Skip to main content

Full text of "On the sensations of tone as a physiological basis for the theory of music"

See other formats


This  is  a  digital  copy  of  a  book  that  was  preserved  for  generations  on  library  shelves  before  it  was  carefully  scanned  by  Google  as  part  of  a  project 
to  make  the  world's  books  discoverable  online. 

It  has  survived  long  enough  for  the  copyright  to  expire  and  the  book  to  enter  the  public  domain.  A  public  domain  book  is  one  that  was  never  subject 
to  copyright  or  whose  legal  copyright  term  has  expired.  Whether  a  book  is  in  the  public  domain  may  vary  country  to  country.  Public  domain  books 
are  our  gateways  to  the  past,  representing  a  wealth  of  history,  culture  and  knowledge  that's  often  difficult  to  discover. 

Marks,  notations  and  other  marginalia  present  in  the  original  volume  will  appear  in  this  file  -  a  reminder  of  this  book's  long  journey  from  the 
publisher  to  a  library  and  finally  to  you. 

Usage  guidelines 

Google  is  proud  to  partner  with  libraries  to  digitize  public  domain  materials  and  make  them  widely  accessible.  Public  domain  books  belong  to  the 
public  and  we  are  merely  their  custodians.  Nevertheless,  this  work  is  expensive,  so  in  order  to  keep  providing  this  resource,  we  have  taken  steps  to 
prevent  abuse  by  commercial  parties,  including  placing  technical  restrictions  on  automated  querying. 

We  also  ask  that  you: 

+  Make  non-commercial  use  of  the  files  We  designed  Google  Book  Search  for  use  by  individuals,  and  we  request  that  you  use  these  files  for 
personal,  non-commercial  purposes. 

+  Refrain  from  automated  querying  Do  not  send  automated  queries  of  any  sort  to  Google's  system:  If  you  are  conducting  research  on  machine 
translation,  optical  character  recognition  or  other  areas  where  access  to  a  large  amount  of  text  is  helpful,  please  contact  us.  We  encourage  the 
use  of  public  domain  materials  for  these  purposes  and  may  be  able  to  help. 

+  Maintain  attribution  The  Google  "watermark"  you  see  on  each  file  is  essential  for  informing  people  about  this  project  and  helping  them  find 
additional  materials  through  Google  Book  Search.  Please  do  not  remove  it. 

+  Keep  it  legal  Whatever  your  use,  remember  that  you  are  responsible  for  ensuring  that  what  you  are  doing  is  legal.  Do  not  assume  that  just 
because  we  believe  a  book  is  in  the  public  domain  for  users  in  the  United  States,  that  the  work  is  also  in  the  public  domain  for  users  in  other 
countries.  Whether  a  book  is  still  in  copyright  varies  from  country  to  country,  and  we  can't  offer  guidance  on  whether  any  specific  use  of 
any  specific  book  is  allowed.  Please  do  not  assume  that  a  book's  appearance  in  Google  Book  Search  means  it  can  be  used  in  any  manner 
anywhere  in  the  world.  Copyright  infringement  liability  can  be  quite  severe. 

About  Google  Book  Search 

Google's  mission  is  to  organize  the  world's  information  and  to  make  it  universally  accessible  and  useful.  Google  Book  Search  helps  readers 
discover  the  world's  books  while  helping  authors  and  publishers  reach  new  audiences.  You  can  search  through  the  full  text  of  this  book  on  the  web 


at|http  :  //books  .  google  .  com/ 


v^' 


.,    •/ 


:i:'.^^^'^'-i-^^^ 


Q.. 


■o 


-s 


»    : 


.<■•' 


.-^ 


V           ■:*    . 

/      s 

) 

^■'-  ,  :' 

.>. 

^■>>.''  -.i 

-'■'■:> 

y 

.  .1 


A 


<-i' 


•s 


'o 


^y 


<0^        /■;:      ::-.''' 


■  i  /•  :  ^ 


y-\  ^:'  ,^J  ^-'""^ 


.<"-• 


r.f/^n 


•■:■>-' V\ 


r-/   <:^   \t\ 


0/    ..     , 


O-'.. 


.<;' 


■•'v 


Jy' 


'vS> 


':,.••  ->: 


V. 


>/ 


'•^.v 


'<^. 


^>, 


'-<< 


^. 


o 


.A 


.<>" 


<••  "*  ^- 


^ 


^^ 


<>' 


6S/  ■■" 


c 


^^  ^^-: 


.< 


^f    /'  ^t:    N  ::^ 


v^^       '0 


U.-\ 


■:'-'!i 


C9        ^/.V.-,--„:y-^^        >^ 


Digitized  |y^©Ojgt€'^ 


JT  t       .^'     ./ 


'    .^ 


^'1, 


^\ 


y  ,1  '■   ; 


O 


h'     A' 


o^ 


■ '   I 

'  ;■■/ 


> 


,N^' 


.<" 


o. 


.^-^^ 


'^^ 


^v 


> 


'f^. 


'-'•    y 


< 


t^\    -i-     '•-•/ 


vV* 


O^ 


.:a 


cr>" 


Cw»  ^^    '■"•    ■^^  V 


../f-r;^.' V  v^ 


m 


l-'';"J 


<>. 


■•s>. 


.^ 


X 


y 


i'3'     ''•5     t-r-'A 


vS^ 


;i"l',^/',.,  !•-./- 


> 


.^\      'O 


<^ 


■\ 


o 


V  '-\      --^      /••  li  1 


< 


«s> 


.-.K      -7.. 


".^>^ 


Digitized  by 


Google 


Digitized  by 


Google 


Digitized  by 


Google 


THE 

SENSATIONS    OF    TONE 


Digitized  by 


Google 


PRIN'TKD    BY 

BromawooDS  asd  co.,  NKW^rnKnr  uguAiiK 

LONDON 


Digitized  by 


Google 


ON    THE 


SENSATIONS   OF   TONE 


AS    A    PHTSIOLOGICAL    BASIS    FOR    THE 


THEOEY     OF    MUSIC 


BY 

HERMANN  L  R  HELMHOLTZ,  M.D. 

FORKION   MEUBEB  OF  THB   BOTAL  SOCUSTIB8   OP   LONDON   AND    BDINBUROH 

FOH1EB8LT   PBOFBSSOB  OF  PHTSIOLOnT    IN   TUB   UNIYBBSITT   OF  HBIDBLBBBG,   AND 

MOW  PBOFE380B  OF  PHYSICS  IN   THE   UNIYBBSITT  OF   BKBLIN 


SECOND    ENGLISH    EDITION 

Translatedj  thorcmghly  Revised  atid  CorrecjUd,  refudered  conformable  to  the  Fourth 

(and  Uut)  German  Edition  of  1877,  tf>ith  numero^is  additional  Notes  and  a 

New  additional  Appendix  bringing  down  information  to  1886, 

arid  espedaUy  adapted  to  tJie  use  of  Musical  Students 


ALEXANDER    J.    ELLIS 

B.A.  P.B.a  F.S.A.  F.CPA  P.CP. 

TWICB  FBB91DENT  OF  THB  PHILOLOGICAL  80CIETT,   MEMBKB  OF  THB   UATHBMATICAL   SOCUTT, 

POBHEBLT    8CUOLAB  OF  TBINITT   COLLBOB,   CAUBBIDOB, 

AUTHOB  OF  '  BABLlr  ENGLISH  FBOMUNCUTION*  AND  *  ALGEBBA  IDENTIFIED  WITH  OBOMETBT ' 


LONDOSr 

LONGMANS,     GREEN,     AND     CO. 

1885 
3 


AA<  '-^CS 


k  U  Digitized  by  Google 

ill    righttVeierhd^  *^'^ 


HH7fi>- 


/\sfl^ 


Digitized  by 


Google 


TEANSLATOE'S   NOTICE 

TO  THB 

SECOND  ENGLISH  EDITION. 


In  preparing  a  new  edition  of  this  translation  of  Professor  Helmhol^z's  great  work  on 
the  Sensations  of  Tone,  which  was  originally  made  from  the  third  German  edition 
of  1870,  and  was  finished 'in  June  1875,  my  first  care  was  to  make  it  exactly 
conform  to  tiiefov/rth  Qennan  edition  of  1877  (the  last  which  has  appeared).  The 
nomerons  alterations  nmde  in  the  fourth  edition  are  specified  in  the  Author's  pre- 
bce.  In  order  that  no  merely  verbal  changes  might  escape  me,  every  sentence 
of  my  translation  was  carefully  re-read  with  the  German.  This  has  enabled  me 
to  correct  several  misprints  and  mistranslations  which  had  escaped  my  previous 
very  careful  revision,  and  I  have  taken  the  opportunity  of  improving  the  language 
in  many  places.    Scarcely  a  page  has  escaped  such  changes. 

Professor  Helmholtz's  book  having  taken  its  place  as  a  work  which  all  candidates 
for  musical  degrees  are  expected  to  study,  my  next  care  was  by  supplementary 
notes  or  brief  insertions,  always  carefully  ^stinguished  from  the  Author's  by  being 
inclosed  in  [  ],  to  explain  any  difficulties  which  the  student  might  feel,  and  to  shew 
him  how  to  acquire  an  insight  into  the  Author's  theories,  which  were  quite  strange 
to  musicians  when  they  appeared  in  the  first  German  edition  of  1863,  ^^^  ui  the 
twenty-two  years  which  have  since  elapsed  have  been  received  as  essentially  valid 
by  those  competent  to  pass  judgment. 

For  this  purpose  I  have  contrived  the  Harmonical,  explained  on  pp.  466-469, 
by  which,  as  shewn  in  numerous  footnotes,  almost  every  point  of  theory  can  be 
illustrated ;  and  I  have  arranged  for  its  being  readily  procurable  at  a  moderato 
charge.  It  need  scarcely  be  said  that  my  interest  in  this  instrument  is  purely 
scientific. 

My  own  Appendix  has  been  entirely  re- written,  much  has  been  rejected  and  the 
rest  condensed,  but,  as  may  be  seen  in  the  Contents,  I  have  added  a  considerable 
imount  of  information  about  points  hitherto  little  known,  such  as  the  Determi- 
nation and  History  of  Musical  Pitch,  Non-Harmonic  scales.  Tuning,  &o.,  and  in 
especial  I  have  given  an  account  of  the  work  recently  done  on  Beats  and  Com- 
binational Tones,  and  on  Vowel  Analysis  and  Synthesis,  mostly  since  the  fourth 
German  edition  appeared. 

Finally,  I  wish  gratefully  to  acknowledge  the  assistance,  sometimes  very  great, 
which  I  have  received  from  Messrs.  D.  J.  Blaikley,  B.  H.  M.  Bosanquet,  Colin 
Brown,  A.  Cavaill6-Coll,  A.  J.  Hipkins,  W.  Huggins,  F.E.S.,  Shuji  Isawa,  H. 
Wird  Poole,  B.  S.  Bockstro,  Hermann  Smith,  Steinway,  Augustus  Stroh,  and 
James  Paul  White,  as  will  be  seen  by  referring  to  their  names  in  the  Index. 

ALEXANDER  J.  ELLIS. 

25  Argyll  Road,  Kexri>:gton: 

J^^y  l8«5.  Digitized  by  GoOglC 


AUTHOR'S    PREFACE 

TO  THE 

FIRST  GERMAN  EDITION. 


In  laying  before  the  Public  the  result  of  eight  years'  labour,  I  must  first  pay  a 
debt  of  gratitude.  The  following  investigations  could  not  have  been  accomplished 
without  the  construction  of  new  instruments,  which  did  not  enter  into  the  inventory 
of  a  Physiological  Institute,  and  which  far  exceeded  in  cost  the  usual  resources  of 
a  German  philosopher.  The  means  for  obtaining  them  have  come  to  me  from 
unusual  sources.  The  apparatus  for  the  artificial  construction  of  vowels,  described 
on  pp.  121  to  126, 1  owe  to  the  munificence  of  his  Majesty  King  Maximilian  of 
Bavaria,  to  whom  German  science  is  indebted,  on  so  many  of  its  fields,  for  ever- 
ready  sympathy  and  assistance.  For  the  construction  of  my  Harmonium  in 
perfectly  natural  intonation,  described  on  p.  316, 1  was  able  to  use  the  Soemmering 
prize  which  had  been  awarded  me  by  the  Senckenberg  Physical  Society  (die 
Senckenbergische  naturforschende  Gesellschaft)  at  Frankfurt-on-the-Main.  While 
publicly  repeating  the  expression  of  my  gratitude  for  this  assistance  in  my  investi- 
gations, I  hope  that  the  investigations  themselves  as  set  forth  in  this  book  will 
prove  far  better  than  mere  words  how  earnestly  I  have  endeavoured  to  make  a 
worthy  use  of  the  means  thus  placed  at  my  command. 

H.  HELMHOLTZ. 

Hbidelbebg  :  October  1862. 


AUTHOR'S   PREFACE 

TO  THE 

THIRD  GERMAN  EDITION. 


Thb  present  Third  Edition  has  been  much  more  altered  in  some  parts  than  the 
second.  Thus  in  the  sixth  chapter  I  have  been  able  to  make  use  of  the  new 
physiological  and  anatomical  researches  on  the  ear.  This  has  led  to  a  modification 
of  my  view  of  the  action  of  Corti's  arches.  Again,  it  appears  that  the  peculiar 
articulation  between  the  auditory  ossicles  called  '  hammer '  and  *  anvil  *  might  easily 
cause  within  the  ear  itself  the  formation  of  harmonic  upper  partial  tones  for  simple 
tones  which  are  sounded  loudly.  By  this  means  that  peculiar  series  of  upper  partial 
tones,  on  the  existence  of  which  the  present  theory  of  music  is  essentially  founded, 
receives  a  new  subjective  value,  entirely  independent  of  external  alterations  in 
the  quality  of  tone.  To  illustrate  the  anatomical  descriptions,  I  have  been  able 
to  add  a  series  of  new  woodcuts,  principally  from  Henle's  Manual  of  Anatomy, 
with  the  author's  permission,  for  which  I  here  take  the  opportunity  of  publicly 
thanking  him. 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


PREFACE.  vii 

I  have  made  many  changes  in  re-editing  the  section  on  the  History  of  Music, 
and  hope  that  I  have  improved  its  connection.  I  must,  however,  request  the 
reader  to  regard  this  section  as  a  mere  compilation  from  secondary  sources ;  I 
have  neither  time  nor  preliminary  knowledge  sufficient  for  original  studies  in  this 
extremely  difficult  field.  The  older  history  of  music  to  the  commencement  of 
Discant,  is  scarcely  more  than  a  confused  heap  of  secondary  subjects,  while  we 
can  only  make  hypotheses  concerning  the  principal  matters  in  question.  Of 
coarse,  however,  every  theory  of  music  must  endeavour  to  bring  some  order  into 
this  chaos,  and  it  cannot  be  denied  that  it  contains  many  important  facts. 

For  the  representation  of  pitch  in  just  or  natural  intonation,  I  have  abandoned 
the  method  originally  proposed  by  Hauptmann,  which  was  not  sufficiently  clear  in 
involved  cases,  and  have  adopted  the  system  of  Herr  A.  von  Oettingen  [p.  276], 
as  had  already  been  done  in  M.  G.  Gu6roult's  French  translation  of  this  book. 

[A  comparison  of  the  Third  with  the  Second  editions,  shewing  the  changes  and  additions 
individually,  is  here  omitted.] 

If  I  may  be  allowed  in  conclusion  to  add  a  few  words  on  the  reception  expe- 
rienced by  the  Theory  of  Music  here  propounded,  I  should  say  that  pubHshed 
objections  almost  exclusively  relate  to  my  Theory  of  Consonance,  as  if  this  were 
the  pith  of  the  matter.  Those  who  prefer  mechanical  explanations  express  their 
regret  at  my  having  left  any  room  in  this  field  for  the  action  of  artistic  invention 
and  esthetic  inclination,  and  they  have  endeavoured  to  complete  my  system  by 
new  numerical  speculations.  Other  critics  with  more  metaphysical  procUvities 
have  rejected  my  Theory  of  Consonance,  and  with  it,  as  they  imagine,  my  whole 
Theory  of  Music,  as  too  coarsely  mechanical. 

I  hope  my  critics  will  excuse  me  if  I  conclude  from  the  opposite  nature  of 
their  objections,  that  I  have  struck  out  nearly  the  right  path.  As  to  my  Theory 
of  Consonance,  I  must  claim  it  to  be  a  mere  systematisation  of  observed  facts 
(with  the  exception  of  the  functions  of  the  cochlea  of  the  ear,  which  is  moreover 
an  hypothesis  that  may  be  entirely  dispensed  with).  But  I  consider  it  a  mistake 
to  make  the  Theory  of  Consonance  the  essential  foundation  of  the  Theory  of 
Music,  and  I  had  thought  that  this  opinion  was  clearly  enough  expressed  in  my  book. 
The  essential  basis  of  Music  is  Melody.  Harmony  has  become  to  Western  Euro- 
peans during  the  last  three  centuries  an  essential,  and,  to  our  present  taste, 
indispensable  means  of  strengthening  melodic  relations,  but  finely  developed 
music  existed  for  thousands  of  years  and  still  exists  in  ultra-European  nations, 
without  any  harmony  at  all.  And  to  my  metaphysico-esthetical  opponents  I  must 
reply,  that  I  cannot  think  I  have  undervalued  the  artistic  emotions  of  the  human 
mind  in  the  Theory  of  Melodic  Construction,  by  endeavouring  to  estabUsh  the 
physiological  facts  on  which  esthetic  feeling  is  based.  But  to  those  who  think  I 
have  not  gone  far  enough  in  my  physical  explanations,  I  answer,  that  in  the  first 
place  a  natural  philosopher  is  never  bound  to  construct  systems  about  everything  he 
knows  and  does  not  know ;  and  secondly,  that  I  should  consider  a  theory  which 
claimed  to  have  shewn  that  all  the  laws  of  modem  Thorough  Bass  were  natural 
necessities,  to  stand  condemned  as  having  proved  too  much. 

Musicians  have  found  most  fault  with  the  manner  in  which  I  have  characterised 
the  Minor  Mode.  I  must  refer  in  reply  to  those  very  accessible  documents,  the 
musical  compositions  of  a.d.  1500  to  a.d.  1750,  during  which  the  modern  Minor 
was  developed.  These  will  shew  how  slow  and  fluctuating  was  its  development, 
and  that  the  last  traces  of  its  incomplete  state  are  still  visible  in  the  works  of 
Sebastian  Bach  and  Handel. 


Heidklbrbg  :  May  1870. 

Digitized  by  VjOOQIC 


AUTHOE'S    PEEFACE 

TO  THB 

FOURTH  GERMAN  EDITION. 


In  the  essential  conceptions  of  masical  relations  I  have  fonnd  nothing  to  alter  in 
this  new  edition.  In  this  respect  I  can  but  maintain  what  I  have  stated  in  the 
chapters  containing  them  and  in  my  preface  to  the  third  [German]  edition.  In 
details,  however,  much  has  been  remodelled,  and  in  some  parts  enlarged.  As  a 
guide  for  readers  of  former  editions,  I  take  the  liberty  to  enumerate  the  following 
places  containing  additions  and  alterations.* 

P.  i6dt  note  *.— On  the  French  Bystem  of  counting  vibrations. 

P.  i8a. — Appunn  and  Preyer,  limits  of  the  highest  audible  tones. 

Pp-  59&  to  656. — On  the  circumstances  under  which  we  distinguish  compound  sensations. 

P.  76a,  b,  c. — Cknnparison  of  the  upper  partial  tones  of  the  strings  on  a  new  and  an  old 
grand  pianoforte. 

P.  83,  note  f.— Herr  Clement  Neumann's  observations  on  the  vibrational  form  of  violin 
strings. 

Pp.  89a  to  936.— The  action  of  blowing  organ-pipes. 

P.  1 10&. — Distinction  of  Ou  from  U. 

Pp.  1 1 16  to  ii6a. — The  various  modifications  in  the  sounds  of  vowels. 

P.  145a. — The  ampulla  and  semicircular  canals  no  longer  considered  as  parts  of  the  organ 
of  hearing. 

P.  1 476. — Waldeyer*s  and  Preyer's  measurements  adopted. 

Pp.  1506  to  I  Sid, — On  the  parts  of  the  ear  which  perceive  noise. 

P.  159&.— Eoenig's  observations  on  combinational  tones  with  tuning-forks. 

P.  I76<2,  note. — ^Preyer's  observations  on  deepest  tones. 

P.  179c. — Preyer's  observation  on  the  sameness  of  the  quality  of  tones  at  the  highest  pitches. 

Pp.  2036  to  204a.— Beats  between  upper  partials  of  the  same  compound  tone  condition  the 
preference  of  musical  tones  with  harmonic  upper  partials. 

Pp.  328c  to  3296.— Division  of  the  Octave  into  53  degrees.    Bosanquet's  harmonium. 

Pp.  338c  to  3396.— Modulations  through  chords  composed  of  two  major  Thirds. 

P.  365,  note  f.— Oettingen  and  Biemann's  theory  of  the  minor  mode. 

P.  372. — Improved  electro-magnetic  driver  of  the  siren. 

P.  373a.— Theoretical  formulaa  for  the  pitch  of  resonators. 

P.  3744;. — Use  of  a  soap-bubble  for  seeing  vibrations. 

Pp.  389^2  to  3966.— Later  use  of  striking  reeds.    Theory  of  the  blowing  of  pipes. 

Pp.  403c  to  405&. — Theoretical  treatment  of  sympathetic  resonance  for  noises. 

P.  417^.— A.  Mayer's  experiments  on  the  audibility  of  vibrations. 

P.  428c,  (2.— Against  the  defenders  of  tempered  intonation. 

P.  429.— Plan  of  Bosanquet's  Harmonium. 


H.  HELMHOLTZ. 


*  [The  pages  of  this  edition  are  substituted      first  edition  of  this  translation  are  mostly 
for  the  German  throughout  these  prefaces,      pointed  out  in  footnotes  as  they  arise.— Trans- 


BsBLiM :  April  1877. 

^  [The  pages  of  this  c 
the  German  throug 
and  omissions  or  alterations  as  respects  the      lator.] 


Digitized  by 


Google 


CONTENTS. 


\*  All  passages  and  notes  in  [  ]  are  due  to  the  Translator,  and  the  Author  is  in  no 
way  responsible  for  their  contents. 

Tbanblatob*8  NonoB  to  thb  Second  Emolibh  Edition,  p.  v. 

Authob's  Pbbvacb  to  thb  Fibst  Gbbuan  Edition,  p.  vi. 

AuTHOB*B  Pbxfjlcb  TO  THB  Thibd  Gebuan  Edition,  pp.  vl-vii. 

Authob's  Pbxface  to  thb  Foubth  Gbbuan  Edition,  p.  viii. 

CONTBNTS,  p.  iz. 

List  or  Fioubbs,  p.  zv. 

List  of  Passages  in  Musical  Notes,  p.  zvi. 

List  of  Tables,  p.  zvii. 

INTBODUCTION,  pp.  i-6. 

Belation  of  Musical  Science  to  Acoustics,  i 

Distinction  between  Physical  and  Physiological  Acoustics,  3 

Plan  of  the  Investigation,  4 


PART  L  (pp.  7-151.) 

ON    THE    COMPOSITION    OF    VIBRATIONS. 
Upper  Pa/rtial  T&nea^  and  QuaUties  of  Tone. 
CHAPTER  I.    On  the  Sensation  of  Sound  in  General,  pp.  8-25. 

Distinction  between  Noise  and  Musical  Tone,  8 

Musical  Tone  due  to  Periodic,  Noise  to  non-Periodic  Motions  in  the  air,  8 

General  Property  of  Undulatory  Motion :  while  Waves  continually  advance,  the  Particles 

of  the  Medium  through  which  they  pass  execute  Periodic  Motions,  9 
Differences  in  Musical  Tones  due  to  Force,  Pitch,  and  Quality,  10 
Force  of  Tone  depends  on  Amplitude  of  Oscillation,  Pitch  on  the  length  of  the  Period  of 

Oscillation,  10- 14 
Simple  relations  of  Vibrational  Numbers  for  the  Consonant  Intervals,  14 
Vibrational  Numbers  of  Consonant  Intervals  calculated  for  the  whole  Scale,  17 
Quality  of  Tone  must  depend  on  Vibrational  Form,  19 
Conception  of  and  Graphical  Representation  of  Vibrational  Form,  20 
Harmonic  Upper  Partial  Tones,  22 
Terms  explained :  Tone,  Musical  Tone,  Simple  Tone,  Partial  Tone,  Compound  Tone,  Pitch 

of  Compound  Tone,  23 

CHAPTER  n.    On  the  Composition  op  Vibrations,  pp.  25-36. 

Comi>osition  of  Waves  illustrated  by  waves  of  water,  25 

The  Heights  of  Superimposed  Waves  of  Water  are  to  be  added  algebraically,  27 

Corresponding  Superimposition  of  Waves  of  Sound  in  the  air,  28 


Digitized  by 


Google 


X  CONTENTS. 

A  Composite  Mass  of  Musical  Tones  will  give  rise  to  a  Periodic  Vibration  when  their  Pitch 

Numbers  are  Multiples  of  the  same  Number,  30 
Every  such  Composite  Mass  of  Tones  may  be  considered  to  be  composed  of  Simple 

Tones,  33 
This  Composition  corresponds,  according  to  G.  S.  Ohm,  to  the  Composition  of  a  Musical 

Tone  from  Simple  Partial  Tones,  33 

CHAPTEE  in.    Analysis  op  Musical  Tones  by  Sympathetic  Resonance, 
pp.  36-49. 

Explanations  of  the  Mechanics  of  Sympathetic  Vibration,  36 

Sympathetic  Resonance  occurs  when  the  exciting  vibrations  contain  a  Simple  Vibration 

corresponding  to  one  of  the  Proper  Vibrations  of  the  Sympathising  Body,  38 
Difference  in  the  Sympathetic  Resonance  of  Tuning-forks  and  Membranes,  40 
Description  of  Resonators  for  the  more  accurate  Analysis  of  Musical  Tones,  43 
Sympathetic  Vibration  of  Strings,  45 
Objective  existence  of  Partial  Tones,  48 

CHAPTER  IV.    On  the  Analysis  of  Musical  Tones  by  the  Ear,  pp.  49-65. 

Methods  for  observing  Upper  Partial  Tones,  49 

Proof  of  G.  S.  Ohm's  Law  by  means  of  the  tones  of  Plucked  Strings,  of  the  Simple  Tones 
of  Tuning-forks,  and  of  Resonators,  51 

Difference  between  Compound  and  Simple  Tones,  56 

Seebeck's  Objections  against  Ohm*s  Law,  58 

The  Difficulties  experienced  in  perceiving  TJpper  Partial  Tones  analytically  depend  upon  a 
peculiarity  common  to  all  human  sensations,  59 

We  practise  observation  on  sensation  only  to  the  extent  necessary  for  clearly  apprehend- 
ing the  external  world,  62 

Analysis  of  Compound  Sensations,  63 

CHAPTER  V.    On  the  Differences  in  the  Quality  op  Musical  Tones, 
pp.  65-119. 

Noises  heard  at  the  beginning  or  end  of  Tones,  such  as  Consonants  in  Speech,  or  during 
Tones,  such  as  Wind- rushes  on  Pipes,  not  included  in  the  Musical  Quality  of  Tone, 
which  refers  to  the  uniformly  continuous  musical  sound,  65 

Limitation  of  the  conception  of  Musical  Quality  of  Tone,  68 

Investigation  of  the  Upper  Partial  Tones  which  are  present  in  different  Musical  Qualities 
of  Tone,  69 

1.  Musical  Tones  without  Upper  Partials,  69 

2.  Musical  Tones  with  Inharmonic  Upper  Partials,  70 

3.  Musical  Tones  of  Strings,  74 

Strings  excited  by  Striking,  74 

Theoretical  Intensity  of  the  Partial  Tones  of  Strings,  79 

4.  Musical  Tones  of  Bowed  Instruments,  80 

5.  Musical  Tones  of  Flute  or  Flue  Pipes,  88 

6.  Musical  Tones  of  Reed  Pipes,  95 

7.  Vowel  Qualities  of  Tone,  103 

Results  for  the  Character  of  Musical  Tones  in  general,  1 18 

CHAPTER  VI.    On  the  Apprehension  of  Qualities  of  Tone,  pp.  119-151. 

Does  Quality  of  Tone  depend  on  Difference  of  Phase  ?  1 19 

Electro-magnetic  Apparatus  for  answering  this  question,  121 

Artificial  Vowels  produced  by  Tuning-forks,  123 

How  to  produce  Difference  of  Phase,  125 

Musical  Quality  of  Tone  independent  of  Difference  of  Phase,  126 

Artificial  Vowels  produced  by  Organ  Pipes,  128 

The  Hypothesis  that  a  Series  of  Sympathetical  Vibrators  exist  in  the  ear,  explains  its 
peculiar  apprehension  of  Qualities  of  Tone,  129 

Description  of  the  parts  of  the  internal  ear  which  are  capable  of  vibrating  sympa- 
thetically, 129 

Damping  of  Vibrations  in  the  Ear,  142 

Supposed  Function  of  the  Cochlea,  145  ^.^.^.^^^  ^^  GoOqIc 


CONTENTS.  xi 

PART  11.  (pp.  152-233.) 

ON     THE     INTERRUPTIONS    OP     HARMONY. 
Combinational  Tones  and  Beats,  Consonance  and  Dissonance. 

CHAPTER  VII.    Combinational  Tones,  pp.  152-159. 

Combinational  Tones  arise  when  Vibrations  which  are  not  of  infinitesimal  magnitude  are 

combined,  152 
Description  of  Combinational  Tones,  153 
Law  determining  their  Pitch  Numbers,  154 
Combinational  Tones  of  different  orders,  155 

Difference  of  the  strength  of  Combmational  Tones  on  different  instruments,  157 
Occasional  Generation  of  Ck>mbinational  Tones  in  the  ear  itself,  158 

CHAPTER  VllI,    On  the  Beats  op  Simple  Tones,  pp.  159-173. 

Interference  of  two  Simple  Tones  of  the  same  pitch,  160 

Description  of  the  Polyphonic  Siren,  for  experiments  on  Interference,  161 

Beinforcement  or  Enfeeblement  of  Sound,  due  to  difference  of  Phase,  163 

Interference  gives  rise  to  Beats  when  the  Pitch  of  the  two  Tones  is  slightly  different,  164 

Law  for  the  Number  of  Beats,  165 

Visible  Beats  on  Bodies  vibrating  sympathetically,  166 

Limits  of  Bapidity  of  Audible  Beats,  167 

CHAPTER  IX.    Deep  and  Deepest  Tones,  pp.  174-179. 

Former  Investigations  were  insufficient,  because  there  was  a  possibility  of  the  ear  being 
deceived  by  Upper  Partial  Tones,  as  is  shewn  by  the  number  of  Beats  on  the  Siren,  174 

Tones  of  less  than  thirty  Vibrations  in  a  second  fall  into  a  Drone,  of  which  it  is  nearly 
or  quite  impossible  to  determine  the  Pitch,  175 
/>        Beats  of  the  Higher  Upper  Partials  of  one  and  the  same  Deep  Compound  Tone,  17S 

CHAPTER  X,    Beats  of  the  Uppeb  Partial  Tones,  pp.  179-197. 

Any  two  Partial  Tones  of  any  two  Compound  Tones  may  beat  if  they  are  sufficiently 
near  in  pitch,  but  if  they  are  of  the  same  pitch  there  will  be  consonance,  179 

Series  of  the  different  Consonances,  in  order  of  the  Distinctness  of  their  Delimitation,  183 

Number  of  Beats  which  arise  from  Mistuning  Consonances,  and  their  effect  in  producing 
Roughness,  184 

Disturbance  of  any  Consonance  by  the  adjacent  Consonances,  186 

Order  of  Consonances  in  respect  to  Harmoniousness,  1S8 

CHAPTER  XI.    Beats  due  to  Combinational  Tones,  pp.  197-21 1. 

The  Differential  Tones  of  the  first  order  generated  by  two  Partial  Tones  are  capable  of 
producing  very  distinct  beats,  197 

Differential  Tones  of  higher  orders  produce  weaker  beats,  even  in  the  case  of  simple  gene- 
rating tones,  199 

Influence  of  Quality  of  Tone  on  the  Harshness  of  Dissonances  and  the  Harmoniousness 
of  Consonances,  205 

CHAPTER  Xn.    Chobds,  pp.  211-233. 

Consonant  Triads,  211 

Major  and  Minor  Triads  distinguished  by  their  Combinational  Tones,  214 
Belative  Harmoniousness  of  Chords  in  different  Inversions  and  Positions,  218 
Betrospeot  on  Preceding  Investigations,  226 


Digitized  by 


Google 


xii  CONTENTS. 


PART  IIL  (pp.  234-371.) 

THE    RELATIONSHIP    OF    MUSICAL    TONES. 
Scalea  and  Tonality, 

CHAPTER  Xin.    Gbnbeal  View  op  the  Different  Principles  op  Musicax* 
Style  in  the  Development  of  Music,  pp.  234-249. 

DiSerenoe  between  the  Physioal  and  the  Esthetioal  Method,  234 

Scales,  Keys,  and  Harmonic  TisBues  depend  upon  esthetio  Principles  of  Style  as  well  fts 

Physical  Causes,  235 
Illustration  from  the  Styles  of  Architecture,  235 
Three  periods  of  Music  have  to  be  distinguished,  236 

1.  Homophonic  Music,  237 

2.  Polyphonic  Music,  244 

3.  Harmonic  Music,  246 

CHAPTER  XIV.    The  Tonality  op  Homophonic  Music,  pp.  250-290. 

Esthetical  Reason  for  Progression  by  Intervals,  250 

Tonal  Relationship  in  Melody  depends  on  the  identity  of  two  partial  tones,  253 

The  Octave,  Fifth,  and  Fourth  were  thus  first  discovered,  253 

Variations  in  Thirds  and  Sixths,  255 

Scales  of  Five  Tones,  used  by  Chinese  and  Gaels,  258 

The  Chromatic  and  Enharmonic  Scales  of  the  Gre^s,  262 

The  Pythagorean  Scales  of  Seven  tones,  266 

The  Greek  and  Ecclesiastical  Tonal  Modes,  267 

Early  Ecclesiastical  Modes,  272 

The  Rational  Construction  of  the  Diatonic  Scales  by  the  principle  of  Tonal  Relationship  in 

the  first  and  second  degrees  gives  the  five  Ancient  Melodic  Scales,  272 
Introduction  of  a  more  Accurate  Notation  for  Pitch,  276 

Peculiar  discovery  of  natural  Thirds  in  the  Arabic  and  Persian  Tonal  Systems,  2S0 
The  meaning  of  the  Leading  Note  and  consequent  alterations  in  the  Modem  Scales,  285 

CHAPTER  XV.    The  Consonant  Chords  of  the  Tonal  Modes,  pp.  290- 
309* 

Chords  as  the  Representatives  of  compound  Musical  Tones  with  peculiar  qualities,  290 
Reduction  of  all  Tones  to  the  closest  relationship  in  the  popular  harmonies  of  the  Major 

Mode,  292  * 

Ambiguity  of  Minor  Chords,  294 

The  Tonic  Chord  as  the  centre  of  the  Sequence  of  Chords,  296 
Relationship  of  Chords  of  the  Scale,  297 
The  Major  and  Minor  Modes  are  best  suited  for  Harmonisation  of  all  the  Ancient  Modes, 

298 
Modem  Renmants  of  the  old  Tonal  Modes,  306 

CHAPTER  XVI.    The  System  op  Keys,  pp.  310-330. 

Relative  and  Absolute  Character  of  the  different  Keys,  310 
Modulation  leads  to  Tempering  the  Intonation  of  the  Intervals,  312 
Hauptmann*s  System  admits  of  a  Simplification  which  makes  its  Realisation  more  Practi- 
cable, 315 
Description  of  an  Harmonium  with  Just  Intonation,  316 
Disadvantages  of  Tempered  Intonation,  322 
Modulation  for  Just  Intonation,  327 


Digitized  by 


Google 


CONTENTS.  xiii 

CHAPTER  XVn.    Of  Discords,  pp.  330-350. 

Ennmeration  of  the  Dissonant  Intervals  in  the  Scale,  331 
Dissonant  Triads,  338 

Chords  of  the  Seventh,  341  "^ 

CfOnoeption  of  the  Dissonant  Note  in  a  Discord,  346 
Discords  as  representatives  of  compoond  tones,  347 

CHAPTER  XVin.    Laws  op  Progression  of  Parts,  pp.  350-362. 

The  Masical  Connection  of  the  Notes  in  a  Melody,  350 

Conseqaent  Bales  for  the  Progression  of  Dissonant  Notes,  353 

Besolation  of  Discords,  354 

Chordal  Sequences  and  Resolution  of  Chords  of  the  Seventh,  355 

Prohihition  of  Consecutive  Fifths  and  Octaves,  359 

Hidden  Fifths  and  Octaves,  361 

False  Relations,  361 

CHAPTER  XIX.    EsTHETicAL  Relations,  pp.  362-371. 

Review  of  Besults  obtained,  362 

Law  of  Unconscious  Order  in  Works  of  Art,  366 

The  Law  of  Melodic  Succession  depends  on  Sensation,  not  on  Consciousness,  368 

And  similarly  for  Consonance  and  Dissonance,  369 

Conclusion,  371 


APPENDICES,  pp.  327-556. 

I.    On  an  Electro-Magnetic  Driving  Machine  for  the  Siren,  372 

II.    On  the  Size  and  Construction  of  Resonators,  372 
III.    On  the  Motion  of  Plucked  Strings,  374 
rv.    On  the  Production  of  Simple  Tones  by  Resonance,  377 

V.    On  the  Vibrational  Forms  of  Pianoforte  Strings,  380 
VL    Analysis  of  the  Motion  of  Violin  Strings,  384 
Vn.    On  the  Theory  of  Pipes,  388 

A.  Influence  of  Resonance  on  Reed  Pipes,  388 

B.  Theory  of  the  Blowing  of  Pipes,  390 
I.  The  Blowing  of  Beed  Pipes,  390 
n.  The  Blowing  of  Flue  Pipes,  394 
[Additions  by  Translator,  396] 

Vni.    Practical  Directions  for  Performing  the  Experiments  on  the  Composition  of  Vowels, 
398 
IX.    On  the  Phases  of  Waves  caused  by  Resonance,  400 
X.    Belation  between  the  Strength  of  Sympathetic  Resonance  and  the  Length  of  Time 

required  for  the  Tone  to  die  away,  405 
XI.    Vibrations  of  the  Membrana  Basilaris  in  the  Cochlea,  406 
XIL    Theory  of  Combinational  Tones,  41 1 
XIIL    Description  of  the  Mechanism  employed  for  opening  the  several  Series  of  Holes  in 

the  Polyphonic  Siren,  413 
XrV.    Variation  in  the  Pitch  of  Simple  Tones  that  Beat,  414 
XV.    Calculation  of  the  Intensity  of  the  Beats  of  Different  Intervals,  41 5 
XVL    On  Beats  of  Combinational  Tones,  and  on  Combinational  Tones  in  the  Siren  and 

Harmonium,  418 
XVn.    Plan  for  Justly-Toned  Instruments  with  a  Single  Manual,  421 
X7IIL    Just  Intonation  in  Singing,  422 
XIX.    Plan  of  Mr.  Bosanquet*s  Manual,  429 
:XX.    Additions  by  the  Translator,  430-556 

*»*  See  separate  Tables  of  Contents  prefixed  to  each  Section. 
[Sect.  A.  On  Temperament,  430 
[Sect.  B.  On  the  Determination  of  Pitch  Numbers,  441 


Digitized  by 


Google 


xiv  CONTENTS. 

[App.  XX-  Additions  by  the  Translator— con^ntt«(f. 

*«*  See  separate  Tables  of  Contents  prefixed  to  each  Section. 
[Sect.  G.  On  the  Caloolation  of  Cents  from  Interval  Batlos,  446 
[S£CT.  D.  Musical  Intervals,  not  exceeding  an  Octave,  arranged  in  order  of  Width, 

451 
[Sect.  E.  On  Musical  Duodenes,  or  the  Development  of  Just  Intonation  for 

Harmony,  457 
[Sect.  F.  Experimental  Instruments  for  exhibiting  the  effects  of  Just  Intonation, 

466 
[Sect.  G.  On  Tuning  and  Intonation,  483 
[Sect.  H.  The  History  of  Musical  Pitch  in  Europe,  493 
[Sect.  E.  Non-Harmonic  Scales,  514 

[Sect.  L.  Becent  Work  on  Beats  and  Combinational  Tones,  527 
[Sect.  M.  Analysis  and  Synthesis  of  Vowel  Sounds,  538 
[Sect.  N.  Miscellaneous  Notes,  544 

[INDEX,  557-576] 


Digitized  by 


Google 


XV 


LIST   OF   FIGUEES. 


13. 
14. 

16. 

17. 
18. 

19. 

20. 

21. 

22. 
23. 

24. 

25. 

26. 

27. 

28. 
29. 

30- 
31. 
32. 


Seebeck*B  Siren,  I  ic  33 

3,  4.  Cagniard  de  la  Tour's  Siren,  126 

Tuning-fork  tracing  its  Curve,  206 

Gtirve  traced  in  Phonautograph,  2od  34. 

Curve  of  Simple  Vibration,  216 

Curve  of  Motion  of  Hammer  moved  by 

Water-wheel,  21c 
Curve  of  Motion  of  Ball  struck  up  on  35. 

its  descent,  21c 
Beprodaotion  of  fig.  7i  23(2  36. 

Curve  shewing  the  Composition  of  a 

simple  Note  and  its  Octave  in  two 

different  phases,  30&,  c  37- 

Curve  shewing  the  Composition  of  a 

simple  note  and  its  Twelfth  in  two 

different  phases,  326  38. 

Tuning-fork  on  Resonance  Box,  40^ 
Forms  of  Vibration  of  a  Circular  Mem-  39. 

brane,  40c,  d 
Pendulum    excited    by    a   membrane 

covering  a  bottle,  42a  40. 

a.  Spherical  Besonator,  436 

b.  Cylindrical  Besonator,  43c  41. 
Forms  of  Vibration  of  Strings,  46a,  b  42. 
Forms  of  Vibration  of  a  String  de-  43. 

fleeted  by  a  Point,  54^,  6 
Action  of  such  a  String  on  a  Sounding- 
board,  S4C  44. 
Bottle  and  Blow-tube  for  producing  a 

simple  Tone,  60c 
Sand  figures  on  circular  elastic  plates,  45. 

71C 
The  Vibration  Microscope,  816  46. 

Vibrations  as   seen  in  the  Vibration 

Microscope,  826 
Vibrational  Forms  for  the  middle  of  a  47* 

Violin  String,  836 
Crumples  on  the  vibrational  form  of  a 

violin  string,  846  48. 

Gradual  development  of  Octave  on  a  '  49. 

violin  string  bowed  near  the  bridge,  50. 

856  .  s«- 

An  open  wooden  and  stoppea  metal  52. 

organ  flue-pipe,  88 
Free  reed  or  Harmonium  vibrator,  956  53- 

Free  and  striking  reed  on  an  organ 

pipe  partly  in  section,  96a,  b  54- 

Membranous  double  reed,  97a 
Reproduction  of  fig.  12,  120a,  6  55- 

Fork  with  electro-magnetic  exciter,  and 

sliding    resonance   box  with  a  lid  56. 

(artificial  vowels),  121b  57- 


Fork  with  electro-magnet  to  serve  as 

interrupter  of  the  current  (artificial 

vowels),  122& 
Appearance  of  figures  seen  through  the 

vibration  microscope  by  two  forks 

when  the  phase  changes   but  the 

tuning  is  correct,  I26d 
The  same  when  the  tuning  is  slightly 

altered,  127a 
Construction  of  the  ear,  general  view, 

with  meatus  auditorius,  labyrinth, 

cochlSa,  and  Eustachian  tube,  129c 
The  three  auditory  ossicles,  hammer, 

anvil,  and  stirrup,  in  their  relative 

positions,  130c 
Two  views  of  the  hammer  of  the  ear, 

1316 
Left  temporal  bone  of  a  newly- born 

child  with  the  auditory  ossicles  in 

sitUy  13IC 
Right    drumskin   with  hammer    seen 

from  the  inside,  131c 
Two  views  of  the  right  anvil,  1330 
Three  views  of  the  right  stirrup,  134a 
A,  left  labyrinth   from  without.     B, 

right  labyrinth  from  within.       C, 

left  labyrinth  from  above,  1366,  c 
Utriculus  and  membranous  semicircular 

canals  (left  side)  seen  from  without, 

137* 
Bony  cochlea  (right  side)  opened  in 

front,  137c,  d 
Transverse    section  of    a  spire  of    a 

cochlSa  which  has    been    softened 

in  hydrochloric  acid,  138a,  b 
Max  Schnitzels  hairs  on  the  internal 

surface  of    the  epithiUum  in    the 

ampulla,  138c,  d 
Expansion  of  the  cochlean  nerve,  139c 
Corti's  membrane,  140a,  6,  c 
Corti's  rods  or  arches  separate,  140^2 
Corti's  rods  or  arches  in  situ,  141 6,  c 
Diagram  of  the  law  of  decrease  of  sym- 
pathetic resonance,  144c,  d 
Interference     of     similarly    disposed 

waves,  1606 
Interference  of    dissimilarly    disposed 

waves,  1 60c 
Lines    of    silence    of    a    tuning-fork, 

1616 
The  Polyphonic  Siren,  162 
Diagram  of  origin  of  beats,  165a 


Digitized  by 


\^oogle 


XVI 


LIST  OP  PASSAGES  IN  MUSICAL  NOTES. 


58.  Phonautographic     representation     of 

beats,  i66a 

59.  Identical  with  fig.  52  bat  now  taken  to 

shew  the  intensity  of  beats  excited 
by  tones  making  different  intervals, 
172c 

60.  A  and  B.    Diagram  of  the  comparative 

ronghness  of  intervals  in  the  first 
and  second  octaves,  1936,  c 

61.  Diagram  of  the  roughness  of  dissonant 

intervals,  333a 

62.  Reproduction  of  fig.  24  A,  p.  3856 

63.  Diagram  of   the   motion  of  a  violin 

string,  387c 


64.  Diagram  of  the  arrangements  for  the 

experiments  on  the  composition  of 
vowels,  3996,  c 

65.  Mechanism  for   opening   the   several 

series  of  holes  in  the  Polyphonic 
Siren,  414a 

66.  Section,  Elevation,  and  Plan  of  Mr. 

Bosanqaet*s  Manual,  429 

In  Additions  by  Translator. 

67.  Perspective  view  of  Mr.  Colin  Brown's 

Fingerboard,  47  id 

68.  Perspective  view,  69  plan,  70  section 

of  Mr.  H.  W.  Poole's  Keyboard,  475 


LIST  OF  PASSAGES  IN  MUSICAL  NOTES. 


The  small  octave,  i$d 

The  once  and  twice  accented  octave,  i6a,  b 

The  great  octave,  166 

The  first  16  Upper  Partials  of  C66,  22c 

The  first  8  Upper  Partials  of  C132,  50a 

Prof.  Helmholtz's  Vowel  Resonances,  1 106 

First  differential  tones  of  the  usual  har- 
monic interval,  1546 

Differential  tones  of  different  orders  of  the 
usual  harmonic  intervals,  1556,  c 

Smnmational  tones  of  the  usual  harmonic 
intervals,  156a 

Examples  of  beating  partials,  i8oc 

Coincident  partials  of  the  principal  con- 
sonant intervals,  iSyi 

Coincident  converted  into  beating  partials 
by  altering  pitch  of  upper  tone,  i86c 

Examples  of  intervals  in  which  a  pair  of 
partials  beat  33  times  in  a  second,  192a 

Major  Triads  with  their  Combinational 
Tones,  215a 

Minor  Triads  with  their  Combinational 
Tones,  2156 

Consonant  Intervals  and  their  Combina- 
tional Tones,  218c 

The  most  Perfect  Positions  of  the  Major 
Triads,  219c 

The  less  Perfect  Positions  of  the  Major 
Triads,  220c 

The  most  Perfect  Positions  of  the  Minor 
Triads,  22 1& 

The  less  Perfect  Positions  of  the  Minor 
Triads,  221c 


The    most    Perfect    Positions   of    Major 

Tetrads  within    the    Compass  of    Two 

Octaves,  223c 
Best  Positions  of  Minor  Tetrads  with  their 

false  Combinational  Tones,  224a 
Ich  bin  spatzieren  gegangen^  2386 
Sic  canta  commat  2396 
Palestrina's  StabcU  Mater,  first  4    bars, 

247c 
Chinese  air  after  Barrow,  260a 
Cockle  ShellSt  older  form,  260& 
Blythe,  blythe,  and  merry  are  we,  261a 
Chinese  temple  hymn  after  Bitschurin,  26 1 6 
Bnus  of  Balqvhidder,  261c 
Five  forms  of  Closing  Chords,  291c 
Two  complete  closes,  293c 
Mode  of  the  Fourth,  three  forms  of  com- 
plete cadence,  302^ 
Concluding  bars  of  S.  Bach's  Chorale,  Was 

mevn  Qott  toiU,  das  gescheh*  alUeit,  304& 
End  of  S.  Bach's  Hymn,  Vem  redemptor 

gentium,  305a 
Doric  cadence  from  And  with  His  stripes 

toe  are  healed,  in  Handel's  Messiah,  307a 
Doric  cadence  from  Hear,  Jacobus  Qod,  in 

Handel's  Samson,  3076 
Examples  of  False  Minor  Triad,  340a 
Examples  of  Hidden  Fifths,  36ic2 
Example  of  Duodenals,  465c 
Mr.'H.  W.  Poole's  method  of  fingering  and 

treatment  of  the  harmonic  Seventh,  477.-1 
Mr.  H.  W.  Poole's  Double  Diatonic  or  Di- 

chordal  Scale  in  0  with  accidentals,  478a 


Digitized  by 


Google 


XVll 


LIST   OF   TABLES. 


Pitch  Nambers  of    Notes  in  Just  Major 

Scftle,  17a 
[Scale  of  Harmonical,  17c,  d] 
[Analogies  of  notes  of  the  piano  and  colours 

of  the  Spectrum,  iSd'] 
Pitch  of  the  different  forms  of  vibration 

of  a  circular  membrane,  41c 
Relative  Pitch  Numbers  of  the  prime  and 

proper  tones  of  a  rod  free  at  both  ends, 

56a 

Proper  Tones  of  circular  elastic  plates,  72a 

Proper  Tones  of  Bells,  72c 

Proper  Tones  of  Stretched  Membranes,  736 

Theoretical  Intensity  of  the  Partial  Tones 
of  Strings,  79c 

[Velocity  in  Sound  in  tubes  of  different 
diameters  —  Blaikley,  qod] 

[Ptrtials  of  Bb  Clarinet— Blaikley,  99c] 

[Harmonics  oi  Eb  horn,  99^] 

[Compass  of  Begisters  of  male  and  female 
voices— Behnke,  loi^ 

Vowel  trigram— Du  Bois  Baymond,  senior, 
1056 

Vowel  Resonances  according  to  Helmholtz 
and  Bonders,  1096 

[Vowel  Resonances  according  to  (i)  Reyher, 
(2)Hellwag,  (3)  Fldrcke,  (4)  Bonders  after 
Helmholtz,  (5)  Bonders  after  Merkel, 
(6)  Hehnholtz,  (7)  Merkel,  (8)  Koenig, 
(9)  Trautmann,  logd] 

Willis's  Vowel  Resonances,  117c 

[Relative  force  of  the  partials  for  producing 
different  vowels,  124^] 

Relation  of  Strength  of  Resonance  to 
Alterations  of  Phase,  125a 

Bifference  of  pitch,  Ac,  necessary  to  reduce 
sympathetic  vibration  to  ^  of  that  pro- 
duced by  perfect  unisonance,  143a 

Nambers  from  which  fig.  52  was  constructed, 
145a 

Measurements  of  the  basilar  membrane  in 
a  new-born  child,  145c 

Alteration  of  size  of  Corti's  rods  as  they 
approach  the  vertex  of  the  cochlga,  i^S'l 

[Preyer's  distinguishable  and  undistinguish  - 
able  intervals,  I47d] 

First  differential  tones  of  the  usual  har- 
monic intervals,  1 54a 

'Differential  tones  of  different  orders  of  the 
usual  harmonic  intervals,  I55<f] 

Different  intervals  which  would  give  33 
beats  of  their  primes,  1720 


[Pitch  numbers  of  Appunn's  bass  reeds, 

1776] 
[Experiments  on  audibility  of   very  deep 

tones,  177c] 
Coincident  partials  for  the  principal  con- 
sonances, 183a 
Pitch  numbers  of  the  primes  which  make 

consonant  intervals  with  a  tone  of  300 

vib.,  184c 
Beating  partials  of  the  notes  in  the  last 

table  with  a  note  of  301  vib.,  and  number 

of  beats,  184^ 
Disturbance  of  a  consonance  by  altering 

one  of  its  tones  by  a  Semitone,  185c 
Influence  of  different  consonances  on  each 

other,  1876 
[Upper  partials  of  a  just  Fifth,  iSSd] 
[Upper  partials  of  an  altered  Fifth,  189c] 
[Comparison  of  the  upper  partials  of  a 

Fourth  and  Eleventh,  major  Sixth  and 

major    Thirteenth,  minor    Sixth     and 

minor  Thirteenth,  189^  and  1906,  c] 
[Comparison  of  the  upper  partials   of   a 

major  and  a  minor  Third,  190^] 
[Comparison  of  the  upper  partials  of  all 

the    usual    consonances,  pointing    out 

those  which  beat,  191 6,  c] 
[Comparison    of    the    upper    partials    of 

septimal    consonances,    involving     the 

seventh  partial,  and  pointing  out  which 

beat,  195c,  d] 
[General  Table  of  the  first  16  harmonics  of 

C66,  shewing  how  they  affect  each  other 

in  any  combination,  197c,  d] 
Table  of  partials  of  200  and  301,  shewing 

their  differential  tones,  198c 
Table  of  possible  triads,  shewing  consonant, 

dissonant,  and  septimal  intervals,  2126,  c 
Table  of  consonant  triads,  214a 
[The  first  16  harmonics  of  C,  21 4(2] 
[Calculation  of  the  Combinational  Tones  of 

the  Major  Triads,  21  ^d] 
[Most  of  the  first  40  harmonics  of  ^,, b ,  2 1 5cl 
[Calculation  of  the  Combinational  Tones  of 

the  Minor  Triads,  215^] 
[Calculation  of  the  Differential  Tones  of 

the  Major  Triads  in  their  most  Perfect 

Positions,  219^2] 
[Calculation  of  the  Combinational  Tones 

of  the  Major  Triads  in  the  less  Perfect 

Positions,  22od] 
[Calculation  of  the  Combinational  Tones  of 
Digitized  by  V^OCK?lC 


XVUl 


LIST   OF  TABLES. 


the  Minor  Triads  in  the  most  and  less 
Perfect  Positions  of  the  Minor  Triads, 
22  Id,  d'] 

[Calculation  of  the  false  Combinational 
Tones  of  Minor  Tetrads  in  their  best 
positions,  224^] 

Ecclesiastical  Modes,  2450,  d 

Partial  Tones  of  the  Tonic,  257a 

[Pentatonic  Scales,  259c,  d] 

[Tetraohords  i  to  8,  with  intervals  in 
cents,  263d'] 

Greek  Diatonic  Scales,  267c 

[Greek  Diatonic  Scales  with  the  intervals 
in  cents,  268c] 

[Greek  Diatonic  Scales  reduced  to  begin- 
ning with  e,  with  the  intervals  in  cents, 
268^1 

Greek  modes  with  the  Greek  Ecclesiastical 
and  Helmholtzian  names,  269a 

Later  Greek  Scale,  270a 

Tonal  Keys,  270c 

Ecclesiastical  Scales  of  Ambrose  of  Milan, 
2716,  c 

The  Five  Melodic  Tonal  Modes,  2726 

[The  Seven  Ascending  and  Descending 
Scales,  compared  with  Greek,  with  inter- 
vals in  cents,  274c,  d] . 

[The  different  scales  formed  by  a  dif- 
ferent choice  of  the  intercalary  tones, 
277c',  d'] 

The  Five  Modes  with  variable  intercalary 
tones,  278a,  b 

[J.  Curwen's  characters  of  the  tones  in 
the  major  scale,  2796,  c] 

[Arabic  Scale  in  relation  to  the  major 
Thirds,  28id'] 

Arabic  Scales,  282&-283C 

[Prof.  Land's  account  of  the  12  Arabic 
Scales,  284  note] 

Five  Modes  as  formed  from  three  chords 
each,  293d,  294a 

The  same  with  double  intercalary  tones, 
297c,  d 

The  same,  final  form,  2986,  c 

Trichordal  Relations  of  the  Tonal  Modes, 

sogd 

[Thirds  and  Sixths  in  Just,   Equal,  and 

Pythagorean  Intonation  compared,  313c] 
[Combinational  Tones  of  Just,  Equal,  and 

Pythagorean  Intonation  compared,  314^] 
The  Chordal  System  of  Prof.  Heimholtz's 

Just  Harmonium,  316c 
[Duodenal  y  statement  of  the  tones  on  Prof. 

Heimholtz's  Just  Harmonium,  317c,  d] 
The  Chordal   System  of  the  minor  keys 

on  Prof.  Heimholtz's  Just  Harmonium, 

318a,  6,  d 
[Table  of  the  relation  of  the  Cycle  of  53  to 

Just  Intonation,  3296,  c] 
Tabular  Expression  of  the  Diagram,  fig. 

bi,332] 
'Table  of  Koughnesi^,  333^'. 


Measurements  of  Glass  Resonators,  373c 

Measurements  of  resonance  tubes  men- 
tioned on  p.  55a,  37 7d 

Table  of  tones  of  a  conical  pipe  of  zinc, 
calculated  from  formula  393c  [with  sub- 
sidiary tables,  393d,  and  394c] 

Table  of  Mayer's  observations  on  numbers 
of  beats,  418a 

Table  of  four  stops  for  a  single  manoal 
justly  intoned  instrument,  421c 

Table  of  five  stops  for  the  same,  422a 

In  the  Additions  by  Translator. 
Table  of  Pythagorean  Intonation,  4336,  e 
Table  of  Meantone  Intonation,  4346 
Table  of  Equal  Intonation,  437c,  d 
Synonymity  of  Eqyal  Temperament,  4386 
Synonymity  of  Mr.  Bosanquet's  Notes  in 

Fifths,  439« 
Notes  of  Mr.  Bosanquet's  Cycle  of  53  in 

order  of  Pitch,  4396,  c,  d 
Expression  of  Just  Intonation  in  the  Cyde 

of  1200,  p.  440 
Principal  Table  for  calculation  of  cents, 

450a,  Auxiliary  Tables,  451a 
Table  of  Intervals  not  exceeding  one  Octave, 

4536 
Unevenly  numbered  Harmonics  up  to  the 

63rd,  457a 
Number  of  any  Interval  not  exceeding  a 

Tritone,  contained  in  an  Octave,  457c 
Harmonic  Duodene  or  Unit  of  Modulation, 

461a 
The  Duodenarlum,  463a 
Fingerboard  of  the  Harmonical,  first  four 

Octaves,  with  scheme,  4676,  fifth  Octave, 

468d 
Just  Harmonium  scheme,  470a 
Just  English  Concertina  scheme,  4706 
Mr.    Colin    Brown's    Voice    Harmonium 

Fingerboard  and  scheme,  471a 
Rev.  Henry  Liston's  Organ  and  scheme, 

473* 
Gen.  Perronet  Thompson's  Organ  scheme, 

473^ 
Mr.  H.  Ward  Poole's  100  tones,  474c 
Mr.  H.  W.  Poole's  scheme  for  keys  of  F, 

C.  G,  476^* 
Mr.    Bosanquet's    Generalised     Keyboard, 

480 
Expression  of  the  degrees  of  the  53  divi< 

sion  by  multiples  of  2,  5  and  7,  p.  481c 
Typographical  Plan  of  Mr.  J.  Paul  White'i 

Fingerboard,  4826 
Specimens  of   tuning  in  Meantone  Tern 

perament,  484c 
Specimens  of  tuning  in  Equal  Tem^Kra 

ment,  4856 
Pianoforte    Tuning-  Fourths   and  Fifths, 

485d 
Cornu    and    Mercadirr's    observaliou    on 

Violin  Intonation,  4S6f  lo  4X76 

Digitized  by^OOQlC 


LIST  OF  TABLES. 


XIX 


Scheme  for  Tuning  in  Eqaal  Temperament, 
4896 

Proof  of  rule  for  Tuning  in  Equal  Tempera- 
ment, 490c,  d 

Proof  of  rule  for  Tuning  in  Meantone  Tem- 
perament, 492a 

Historical  Pitches  in  order  from  Lowest  to 
Highest,  495a  to  504a 

Classified  Index  to  the  last  Table,  5046  to 
5116 

Effects  of  the  length  of  the  foot  in  differ- 
ent countries  on  the  pitch  of  organs, 

Non-harmonic  scales,  514c  to  519c 


Vowel  sound  '  Oh  ! '     Analysis  at  Tarious 

pitches  by  Messrs.  Jenkin  <&  Ewing,  $^gd 

to  541  & 
Vowel  sounds  *oo,'  'awe,'  *ah,'  analysis 

at  various   pitches  by  Messrs.    Jenkin 

and  Ewing,  p.  ^ic,  d 
Mean  and  actual  Compass  of  the  Human 

Voice,  545a,  6,  c 
True  Tritonic,  False  Tritonic,   Zarlino*s, 

Meantone    and    Equal    Temperaments, 

compared,  548a 
Presumed  Characters  of  Major  and  Minor 

Keys,  551a,  6 


Corrigenda. 

v.  loid,  note,  line  la  from  bottom, /<»•  i.  Upper  thick  read  x.  Lower  thick. 

P.  1396  and  eUevhtrt,  cochlean/or  cochlear  U  intentional, 

P.  aSad,  note,  line  10  from  bottom,  after  70*6  cents,  omit  the  remainder  of  the  paragraph, 
and  read  For  the  possible  origin  of  Villoteau's  error  see  in/Hi  p.  5ao6  to  saod'. 

P.  339^',  note  Xt  liae  >7  txova.  bottom, /or  No.  6  read  No.  7. 

P.  356c,  lines  15  and  x6  from  bottom  of  text,  for  o* b  —  c  +  ** b  read  a»b  +  c  —  tf» b . 

P.  356*1,  line  4  from  bottom  of  text,  tat  c  —  e^  —  g  read  c  +  *,  —  jr. 

P.  477,  milsic,  line  a.  bar  a,  dele  the  reference  number  8,  and  the  corresponding  note  below. 

P.  478d',  last  words  of  lines  7  and  5  from  bottom. /or  lightly  and  bad  read  tightly  and  best. 
The  passage  trill  thertifore  read  These  34  lerers  are  a  qoarter  of  an  inch  wide,  and  can 
play  a  pianoforte  with  hammers  half  the  common  width,  with  single  strings,  but 
larger  and  tightly  strained,  so  as  to  yield  the  maximum  tone,  tension  nearly  to 
breaking  point  giving  the  best  tone.' 

P.  501,  col.  1,  for  300  cents  read  330  cents. 

P.  5X9e,  No.  X30, /or  reosen  read  riosen,  and  for  additional  information  on  Japanese  Scales 
generally^  see  p.  556. 


Digitized  by 


Google 


Digitized  by 


Google 


INTEODUCTION. 


In  the  present  work  an  attempt  will  be  made  to  connect  the  boundaries 
of  two  sciences,  which,  although  drawn  towards  each  other  by  many 
natural  aflSnities,  have  hitherto  remained  practically  distinct — I  mean  the 
boundaries  of  physical  and  physiological  acoustics  on  the  one  side,  and  of 
Jmiisical  science  and  esthetics  on  the  other.  The  class  of  readers  addressed 
will,  consequently,  have  had  very  different  cultivation,  and  will  be  affected 
by  very  different  interests.  It  will  therefore  not  be  superfluous  for  the 
author  at  the  outset  distinctly  to  state  his  intention  in  undertaking  the 
work,  and  the  aim  he  has  sought  to  attain.  The  horizons  of  physics, 
philosophy,  and  art  have  of  late  been  too  widely  separated,  and,  as  a 
consequence,  the  language,  the  methods,  and  the  aims  of  any  one  of  these 
studies  present  a  certain  amount  of  diflSculty  for  the  student  of  any  other  % 
of  them ;  and  possibly  this  is  the  principal  cause  why  the  problem  here 
undertaken  has  not  been  long  ago  more  thoroughly  considered  and  advanced 
towards  its  solution. 

It  is  true  that  acoustics  constantly  employs  conceptions  and  names 
bcMTowed  from  the  theory  of  harmony,  and  speaks  of  the  *  scale,'  *  intervals,' 
•consonances,'  and  so  forth;  and  similarly,  manuals  of  Thorough  Bass 
generally  begin  with  a  physical  chapter  which  speaks  of  *  the  numbers  of 
vibrations,'  and  fixes  their  *  ratios '  for  the  different  intervals ;  but,  up  to 
the  present  time,  this  apparent  connection  of  acoustics  and  music  has  been 
wholly  external,  and  may  be  regarded  rather  as  an  expression  given  to  the 
feeling  that  such  a  connection  must  exist,  than  as  its  actual  formulation. 
Physical  knowledge  may  indeed  have  been  useful  for  musical  instrument 
makers,  but  for  the  development  and  foundation  of  the  theory  of  harmony  % 
it  has  hitherto  been  totally  barren.  And  yet  the  essential  facts  within  the 
field  here  to  be  explained  and  turned  to  accoimt,  have  been  known  from  the 
earliest  times.  Even  Pythagoras  (fl.  circa  b.c.  540-510)  knew  that  when 
strings  of  different  lengths  but  of  the  same  make,  and  subjected  to  the 
same  tension,  were  used  to  give  the  perfect  consonances  of  the  Octave, 
Fifth,  or  Fourth,  their  lengths  must  be  in  the  ratios  of  i  to  2,  2  to  3,  or 
3  to  4  respectively,  and  if,  as  is  probable,  his  knowledge  was  partly  derived 
from  the  Egyptian  priests,  it  is  impossible  to  conjecture  in  what  remote 
antiquity  this  law  was  first  known.  Later  physics  has  extended  the  law  of 
Pythagoras  by  passing  from  the  lengths  of  strings  to  the  number  of  vibra- 
^ons,  and  thus  making  it  applicable  to  the  tones  of  all  musical  instruments, 
and  the  numerical  relations  4  to  5  and  5  to  6  have  been  added  to  the  above 

Digitized  by  Vj£>OQ IC 


t  PLAN  OF  THE  WORK.  introd. 

for  the  less  perfect  consonances  of  the  major  and  mmor  Thirds,  but  I  am 
not  aware  that  any  real  step  was  ever  made  towards  answering  the  ques- 
tion :  What  haue  musical  consonances  to  do  with  the  ratios  of  the  first  six 
numbers  ?  Musicians,  as  well  as  philosophers  and  physicists,  have  generally 
contented  themselves  with  saying  in  effect  that  human  minds  were  in  some 
unknown  manner  so  constituted  as  to  discover  the  numerical  relations  of 
musical  vibrations,  and  to  have  a  peculiar  pleasure  in  contemplating  simple 
ratios  which  are  readily  comprehensible. 

Meanwhile  musical  esthetics  has  made  unmistakable  advances  in  those 
points  which  depend  for  their  solution  rather  on  psychological  feeling  than 
on  the  action  of  the  senses,  by  introducing  the  conception  of  movement  in 

^  the  examination  of  musical  works  of  art.  E.  Hanslick,  in  his  book  *  on  the 
Beautiful  in  Music'  (Ueber  das  mvsikalisch  Schone),  triumphantly  attacked 
the  false  standpoint  of  exaggerated  sentimentality,  from  which  it  was 
fashionable  to  theorise  on  music,  and  referred  the  critic  to  the  simple 
elements  of  melodic  movement.  The  esthetic  relations  for  the  structure  of 
musical  compositions,  and  the  characteristic  differences  of  individual  forms 
of  composition,  are  explained  more  fully  in  Vischer's  *  Esthetics '  (Aesthetik). 
In  the  inorganic  world  the  kind  of  motion  we  see,  reveals  the  kind  of  moving 
force  in  action,  and  in  the  last  resort  the  only  method  of  recognising  and 
measuring  the  elementary  powers  of  nature  consists  in  determining  the 
motions  they  generate,  and  this  is  also  the  case  for  the  motions  of  bodies 
or  of  voices  which  take  place  under  the  influence  of  human  feelings.   Hence 

^  the  properties  of  musical  movements  which  possess  a  graceful,  dallying,  or 
a  heavy,  forced,  a  dull,  or  a  powerful,  a  quiet,  or  excited  character,  and  so 
on,  evidently  chiefly  depend  on  psychological  action.  In  the  same  way 
questions  relating  to  the  equilibrium  of  the  separate  parts  of  a  musical 
composition,  to  their  development  from  one  another  and  their  connection 
as  one  clearly  intelligible  whole,  bear  a  close  analogy  to  similar  questions  in 
architecture.  But  all  such  investigations,  however  fertile  they  may  have 
been,  cannot  have  been  otherwise  than  imperfect  and  uncertain,  so  long  as 
they  were  without  their  proper  origin  and  foundation,  that  is,  so  long  as 
there  was  no  scientific  foundation  for  their  elementary  rules  relating  to  the 
construction  of  scales,  chords,  keys  and  modes,  in  short,  to  all  that  is 
usually  contained  in  works  on  '  Thorough  Bass.'   In  this  elementary  region 

^  we  have  to  deal  not  merely  with  unfettered  artistic  inventions,  but  with  the 
natural  power  of  immediate  sensation.  Music  stands  in  a  much  closer 
connection  with  pure  sensation  than  any  of  the  other  arts.  The  latter 
rather  deal  with  what  the  senses  apprehend,  that  is  with  the  images  of 
outward  objects,  collected  by  psychical  processes  from  immediate  sensation. 
Poetry  aims  most  distinctly  of  all  at  merely  exciting  the  formation  of 
images,  by  addressing  itself  especially  to  imagination  and  memory,  and  it 
is  only  by  subordinate  auxiliaries  of  a  more  musical  kind,  such  as  rhythm, 
and  imitations  of  sounds,  that  it  appeals  to  the  immediate  sensation  of 
hearing.  Hence  its  effects  depend  mainly  on  psychical  action.  The  pUisti^; 
arts,  although  they  make  use  of  the  sensation  of  sight,  address  the  eye 
almost  in  the  same  way  as  poetry  addresses  the  ear.  Their  main  purpose 
8  to  excite  in  us  the  image  of  an  external  object  of  determinate  form  and 
colour.     The  spectator  is  essentially  intended  to  interest  himself  in  this 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


iNTROD.  PLAN  OF  THE  WORK.  3 

image,  and  enjoy  its  beauty ;  not  to  dwell  upon  the  means  by  which  it  was 
created.  It  must  at  least  be  allowed  that  the  pleasure  of  a  connoisseur  or 
virtuoso  in  the  constructive  art  shewn  in  a  statue  or  a  picture,  is  not  an 
essential  element  of  artistic  enjoyment. 

It  is  only  in  painting  that  we  find  colour  as  an  element  which  is  directly  j  ^  -  ^* 
appreciated  by  sensation,  without  any  intervening  act  of  the  intellect.  On 
the  contrary,  in  mvmc,  the  sensations  of  tone  are  the  material  of  the  art. 
So  far  as  these  sensations  are  excited  in  music,  we  do  not  create  out  of 
them  any  images  of  external  objects  or  actions.  Again,  when  in  hearing  a 
concert  we  recognise  one  tone  as  due  to  a  violin  and  another  to  a  clarinet, 
our  artistic  enjoyment  does  not  depend  upon  our  conception  of  a  violin  or 
clarinet,  but  solely  on  our  hearing  of  the  tones  they  produce,  whereas  the  ^ 
artistic  enjoyment  resulting  from  viewing  a  marble  statue  does  not  depend 
on  the  white  light  which  it  reflects  into  the  eye,  but  upon  the  mental  image 
of  the  beautiful  human  form  which  it  calls  up.  In  this  sense  it  is  clear  that ' 
music  has  a  more  immediate  connection  with  pure  sensation  than  any  other 
of  the  fine  arts,  and,  consequently,  that  the  theory  of  the  sensations  of 
hearing  is  destined  to  play  a  much  more  important  part  in  musical  esthetics, 
than,  for  example,  the  theory  of  chiaroscuro  or  of  perspective  in  painting. 
Those  theories  are  certainly  useful  to  the  artist,  as  means  for  attaining  the 
most  perfect  representation  of  nature,  but  they  have  no  part  in  the  artistic 
effect  of  his  work.  In  music,  on  the  other  hand,  no  such  perfect  represen- 
tation of  nature  is  aimed  at ;  tones  and  the  sensations  of  tone  exist  for 
themselves  alone,  and  produce  their  effects  independently  of  anything  behind  % 
them. 

This  theory  of  the  sensations  of  hearing  belongs  to  natural  science,  and 
comes  in  the  first  place  under  jyhysiological  acoustics.  Hitherto  it  is  the 
physical  part  of  the  theory  of  sound  that  has  been  almost  exclusively  treated 
at  length,  that  is,  the  investigations  refer  exclusively  to  the  motions  produced 
by  solid,  liquid,  or  gaseous  bodies  when  they  occasion  the  sounds  which  the 
ear  appreciates.  This  physical  acoustics  is  essentially  nothing  but  a  section 
of  the  theory  of  the  motions  of  elastic  bodies.  It  is  physically  indifferent 
whether  observations  are  made  on  stretched  strings,  by  means  of  spirals  of 
bra«8  wire,  (which  vibrate  so  slowly  that  the  eye  can  easily  follow  their 
motions,  and,  consequently,  do  not  excite  any  sensation  of  sound,)  or  by 
means  of  a  violin  string,  (where  the  eye  can  scarcely  perceive  the  vibrations  ^ 
which  the  ear  readily  appreciates).  The  laws  of  vibratory  motion  are  pre- 
cisely the  same  in  both  cases ;  its  rapidity  or  slowness  does  not  affect  the 
laws  themselves  in  the  slightest  degree,  although  it  comjiels  the  observer  to 
apply  different  methods  of  observation,  the  eye  for  one  and  the  ear  for 
the  other.  In  physical  acoustics,  therefore,  the  phenomena  of  hearing  are 
taken  into  consideration  solely  because  the  ear  is  the  most  convenient  and 
handy  means  of  observing  the  more  rapid  elastic  vibrations,  and  the  physicist 
is  compelled  to  study  the  peculiarities  of  the  natural  instrument  which  he  is 
employing,  in  order  to  control  the  correctness  of  its  indications.  In  this 
way,  although  physical  acoustics  as  hitherto  pursued,  has,  undoubtedly, 
collected  many  observations  and  much  knowledge  concerning  the  action  of 
the  ear,  which,  therefore,  belong  to  physiological  acoustics,  these  results  wore 
not  the  principal  object  of  its  investigations ;  they  were  merely  secondary 

Digitized  by  ^^0Ogle 


4  PLAN  OF  THE  WOKK.  introd. 

and  isolated  facts.  The  only  justification  for  devoting  a  separate  chapter 
to  acoustics  in  the  theory  of  the  motions  of  elastic  bodies,  to  which  it 
essentially  belongs,  is,  that  the  application  of  the  ear  as  an  instrument 
of  research  influenced  the  nature  of  the  experiments  and  the  methods  of 
observation. 

But  in  addition  to  a  physical  there  is  a  physiological  theory  of  acoiisticsy 
the  aim  of  which  is  to  investigate  the  processes  that  take  place  within  the 
ear  itself.  The  section  of  this  science  which  treats  of  the  conduction  of  the 
motions  to  which  sound  is  due,  from  the  entrance  of  the  external  ear  to  the 
expansions  of  the  nerves  in  the  labyrinth  of  the  inner  ear,  has  received 
much   attention,   especially  in   Germany,   since   ground  was  broken  by 

H  Johannes  Mueller.  At  the  same  time  it  must  be  confessed  that  not  many 
results  have  as  yet  been  established  with  certainty.  But  these  attempts 
attacked  only  a  portion  of  the  problem,  and  left  the  rest  untouched. 
Investigations  into  the  processes  of  each  of  our  organs  of  sense,  have  in 
general  three  different  parts.  First  we  have  to  discover  how  the  agent 
reaches  the  nerves  to  be  excited,  as  light  for  the  eye  and  sound  for  the  ear. 
This  may  be  called  the  physical  part  of  the  corresponding  physiological 
investigation.  Secondly  we  have  to  investigate  the  various  modes  in  which 
the  nerves  themselves  are  excited,  giving  rise  to  their  various  scfisations^ 
and  finally  the  laws  according  .to  which  these  sensations  result  in  mental 
images  of  determinate  external  objects,  that  is,  in  perceptions.  Hence  we 
have  secondly  a  specially  physiological  investigation  for  sensations,  and 

H  thirdly,  a  specially  psychological  investigation  for  perceptions.  Now  whilst 
the  physical  side  of  the  theory  of  hearing  has  been  already  frequently  ' 
attacked,  the  results  obtained  for  its  physiological  and  psychologix:al  sections 
are  few,  imperfect,  and  accidental.  Yet  it  is  precisely  the  physiological  part 
in  especial — the  theory  of  the  sensations  of  hearing — to  which  the  theory 
of  music  has  to  look  for  the  foundation  of  its  structure. 

In  the  present  work,  then,  I  have  endeavoured  in  the  first  place  to  collect 
and  arrange  such  materials  for  a  theory  of  the  sensations  of  hearing  as  ah*eady 
existed,  or  as  I  was  able  to  add  from  my  own  personal  investigations.  Of 
course  such  a  first  attempt  must  necessarily  be  somewhat  imperfect,  and  be 
hmited  to  the  elements  and  the  most  interesting  divisions  of  the  subject 
discussed.     It  is  in  this  light  that  I  wish  these  studies  to  be  regarded. 

IF  Although  in  the  propositions  thus  collected  there  is  little  of  witirely  new 
discoveries,  and  although  even  such  apparently  new  facts  and  observations 
as  they  contain  are,  for  the  most  part,  more  properly  speaking  the  imme- 
diate consequences  of  my  having  more  completely  carried  out  known 
theories  and  methods  of  investigation  to  their  legitimate  consequences,  and 
of  my  having  more  thoroughly  exhausted  their  results  than  had  heretofore 
been  attempted,  yet  I  cannot  but  think  that  the  facts  frequently  receive  new 
importance  and  new  illumination,  by  being  regarded  from  a  fresh  point  of 
view  and  in  a  fresh  connection. 

The  First  Part  of  the  following  investigation  is  essentially  physical  and 
physiological.  It  contains  a  general  investigation  of  the  phenomenon  of 
harmonic  upper  partial  tones.  The  nature  of  this  phenomenon  is  established, 
and  its  relation  to  quality  of  tone  is  proved.  A  series  of  qualities  of  tone  are 
analysed  in  respect  to  their  harmonic  upper  partial  tones,  and  it  results 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


ixTBOD.  PLAN  OF  THE  WORK.  5 

that  these  upper  partial  tones  are  not,  as  was  hitherto  thought,  isolated 
phenomena  of  small  importance,  but  that,  with  very  few  exceptions,  they 
determine  the  qualities  of  tone  of  almost  all  instruments,  and  are  of  the 
greatest  importance  for  those  qualities  of  tone  which  are  best  adapted  for 
musical  purposes.  The  question  of  how  the  ear  is  able  to  perceive  these  ' 
harmonic  upper  partial  tones  then  leads  to  an  hypothesis  respecting  the 
mode  in  which  the  auditory  nerves  are  excited,  which  is  well  fitted  to  reduce 
all  the  facts  and  laws  in  this  department  to  a  relatively  simple  mechanical 
conception. 

The  Second  Part  treats  of  the  disturbances  produced  by  the  simultaneous 
production  of  two  tones,  namely  the  combinational  tones  and  beats*     The 
physiologico-phy^sical  investigation  shews   that  two  tones  can  be  simul-  H 
taneously  heard  by  the  ear  without  mutual  disturbance,  when  and  onl)^ 
when  they  stand  to  each  other  in  the  perfectly  determinate  and  well-known  co^'*^'^ 
relations  of  intervals  which  form  musical  consonance.     We  are  thus  imme- , 
diately  introduced  into  the  field  of  music  proper,  and  are  led  to  discover 
the  physiological  reason  for  that  enigmatical  numerical  relation  announced 
by  Pythagoras.     The  magnitude  of  the  consonant  intervals  is  independent 
of  the  quality  of  tone,  but  the  harmoniousness  of  the  consonances,  and  the 
distinctness  of  their  separation  from  dissonances,  depend  on  the  quality  of 
tone.    The  conclusions  of  physiological  theory  here  agree  precisely  with  the 
musical  rules  for  the  formation  of  chords ;  they  even  go  more  into  par- 
ticulars than  it  was  possible  for  the  latter  to  do,  and  have,  as  I  believe,  the 
authority  of  the  best  composers  in  their  favour.  f 

In  these  first  two  Parts  of  the  book,  no  attention  is  paid  to  esthetic 
considerations.  Natural  phenomena  obeying  a  blind  necessity,  are  alone 
treated.  The  Third  Part  treats  of  the  construction  of  musical  scab's  and 
Kott's.  Here  we  come  at  once  upon  esthetic  ground,  and  the  diflfcrcnce«  of  ^ 
national  and  individual  tastes  begin  to  appear.  Modern  music  has  especially  | 
developed  the  principle  of  tonality,  which  connects  all  the  tones  in  a  piece 
of  music  by  their  relationship  to  one  chief  tone,  called  the  tonic.  On 
admitting  this  principle,  the  results  of  the  preceding  investigations  furnish 
a  method  of  constructing  our  modern  musical  scales  and  modes,  from  which 
all  arbitrary  assumption  is  excluded. 

I  was  unwilling  to  separate  the  physiological  investigation  from  its 
musical  consequences,  because  the  correctness  of  these  consequences  must  % 
be  to  the  physiologist  a  verification  of  the  correctness  of  the  physical  and 
physiological  views  advanced,  and  the  reader,  who  takes  up  my  book  for  its 
musical  conclusions  alone,  cannot  form  a  perfectly  clear  view  of  the  meaning 
and  bearing  of  these  consequences,  unless  he  has  endeavoured  to  get  at 
least  some  conception  of  their  foundations  in  natural  science.  But  in  order 
to  facilitate  the  use  of  the  book  by  readers  who  have  no  special  knowledge 
of  physics  and  mathematics,  I  have  transferred  to  appendices,  at  the  end 
of  the  book,  all  special  instructions  for  performing  the  more  complicated 
experiments,  and  also  all  mathematical  investigations.  These  appendices 
are  therefore  especially  intended  for  the  physicist,  and  contain  the  proofs 
of  my  assertions.*  In  this  way  I  hope  to  have  consulted  the  interests  of 
both  classes  of  readers. 

*  ^The  additional  Appendix  XX.  by  the  Translator  is  intended  especially  for  the  use  of  t    • 
mn?ica:  %tr\dentsi.— Translator.]  Digitized  by  V^OOQ IC 


««v»t« 


6  PLAN  OF  THE   WORK.  intkod. 

It  is  of  course  impossible  for  any  one  to  understand  the  investigations 
thoroughly,  who  does  not  take  the  trouble  o(  becoming  acquainted  by  per- 
sonal observation  with  at  least  the  fundamental  phenomena  mentioned. 
Fortunately  with  the  assistance  of  common  musical  instruments  it  is  easy 
for  any  one  to  become  acquainted  with  harmonic  upper  partial  tones,  com- 
binational tones,  beats,  and  the  like.*  Personal  observation  is  better  than 
the  exactest  description,  especially  when,  as  here,  the  subject  of  investiga- 
tion is  an  analysis  of  sensations  themselves,  which  are  always  extremely 
difficult  to  describe  to  those  who  have  not  experienced  them. 

In  my  somewhat  unusual  attempt  to  pass  from  natural  philosophy  into 
the  theory  of  the  arts,  I  hope  that  I  have  kept  the  regions  of  physiology 

f  and  esthetics  sufficiently  distinct.  But  I  can  scarcely  disguise  from  myself, 
that  although  my  researches  are  confined  to  the  lowest  grade  of  musical 
grammar,  they  may  probably  appear  too  mechanical  and  unworthy  of  the 
dignity  of  art,  to  those  theoreticians  who  are  accustomed  to  summon  the 
enthusiastic  feelings  called  forth  by  the  highest  works  of  art  to  the  scientific 
investigation  of  its  basis.  To  these  I  would  simply  remark  in  conclusion, 
that  the  following  investigation  really  deals  only  with  the  analysis  of 
actually  existing  sensations — that  the  physical  methods  of  observation 
employed  are  almost  solely  meant  to  facilitate  and  assure  the  work  of  this 
analysis  and  check  its  completeness — and  that  this  analysis  of  the  sensations 
would  suffice  to  furnish  all  the  results  required  for  musical  theory,  even 
independently  of  my  physiological  hypothesis  concerning  the  mechanism  of 

f  hearing,  already  mentioned  (p.  5^),  but  that  I  was  unwilling  to  omit  that 
hypothesis  because  it  is  so  well  suited  to  furnish  an  extremely  simple  con- 
nection between  all  the  very  various  and  very  complicated  phenomena 
which  present  themselves  in  the  course  of  this  investigation.")- 

*  [But  the  use  of  the  flarmontcaZ,  described  London,  Macmillan,  1873.    Such  readers  will 

in  App.  XX.  sect.  F.  No.  i,  and  invented  for  also  find  a  clear  exposition  of  the  physical 

the  purpose  of  illustrating  the  theories  of  this  relations  of  sound  in  J.  Tyndall,  On  Somuit 

work,  is  recommended  as  greatly  superior  for  a  course  of  eight  lectures,  London,  1867,  (the 

students  and  teachers  to  any  other  instrument.  last  or  fourth  edition  1883)  Longmans,  Green, 

— Translator.]  &   Co.    A  German  translation  of  this  work, 

f  Headers  unaccustomed  to  mathematical  entitled  Der  Scliall,  edited  by  H.  Helmholtz 

and    physical    considerations    will    find    an  and  G.  Wiedemann,  was  published  at  Bruns- 

abridged  account  of  the  essential  contents  of  wick  in  1874. 
this  book  in  Sedley  Taylor,  Sound  aiid  Music^ 


*^*  [The  marks  f  in  the  outer  margin  of  each  page,  separate  the  page  into 
4  sections,  referred  to  as  a,  6,  c,  dj  placed  after  the  number  of  the  page.  If  aiiy 
section  is  in  double  columns,  the  letter  of  the  second  column  is  accented,  as 
p.  13d',] 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


PART    I. 

ON  THE  COMPOSITION  OF  VIBRATIONS. 


UPPER  PARTIAL  TONES,  AND  QUALITIES  OF  TONE. 


CHAPTER  L 

ON   THE   SENSATION   OF   SOUND   IN  GENERAL. 

Sensations  result  from  the  action  of  an  external  stimulus  on  the  sensitive  apparatus 
of  our  nerves.  Sensations  differ  in  kind,  partly  with  the  organ  of  sense  excited, 
and  partly  with  the  nature  of  the  stimulus  employed.  Each  organ  of  sense  pro- 
duces peculiar  sensations,  which  cannot  be  excited  by  means  of  any  other ;  the 
eye  gives  sensations  of  light,  the  ear  sensations  of  soimd,  the  skin  sensations  of 
touch.  Even  when  the  same  sunbeams  which  excite  in  the  eye  sensations  of  light, 
impinge  on  the  skin  and  excite  its  nerves,  they  are  felt  only  as  heat,  not  as  light.  % 
In  the  same  way  the  vibration  of  elastic  bodies  heard  by  the  ear,  can  also  be  felt 
by  the  skin,  but  in  that  case  produce  only  a  whirring  fluttering  sensation,  not 
soimd.  ^  The  sensation  of  sound  is  therefore  a  species  of  reaction  against  external 
stimulus,  peculiar  to  the  ear,  and  excitable  in  no  other  organ  of  the  body,  and  is 
completely  distinct  from  the  sensation  of  any  other  sense. 

As  our  problem  is  to  study  the  laws  of  the  sensation  of  hearing,  our  first 
business  will  be  to  examine  how  many  kinds  of  sensation  the  ear  can  generate,  and 
what  differences  in  the  external  means  of  excitement  or  sound,  correspond  to  these 
differences  of  sensation. 

The  first  and  principal  difference  between  various  sounds  experienced  by  our  ear, 
is  that  between  noises  and  mtisical  tones.  The  soughing,  howling,  and  w^histling 
of  the  wind,  the  splashing  of  water,  the  rolling  and  rumbling  of  carriages,  are 
examples  of  the  first  kind,  and  the  tones  of  all  musical  instruments  of  the  second. 
Noises  and  musical  tones  may  certainly  intermingle  in  very  various  degrees,  and  H 
pass  insensibly  into  one  another,  but  their  extremes  are  widely  separated. 

The  nature  of  the  difference  between  musical  tones  and  noises,  can  generally 
be  determined  by  attentive  aural  observation  without  artificial  assistance.  We 
perceive  that  generally,  a  noise  is  accompanied  by  a  rapid  alternation  of  different 
kinds  of  sensations  of  sound.  Think,  for  example,  of  the  rattling  of  a  carriage 
over  granite  paving  stones,  the  splashing  or  seething  of  a  waterfall  or  of  the  waves 
of  the  sea,  the  rustling  of  leaves  in  a  wood.  In  all  these  cases  we  have  rapid, 
irregular,  but  distinctly  perceptible  alternations  of  various  kinds  of  sounds,  which 
crop  up  fitfully.  When  the  wind  howls  the  alternation  is  slow,  the  sound  slowly 
and  gradually  rises  and  then  falls  again.  It  is  also  more  or  less  possible  to  separate 
restlessly  alternating  sounds  in  case  of  the  greater  number  of  other  noises.  We 
shall  hereafter  become  acquainted  with  an  instrument,  called  a  resonator,  which 
will  materially  assist  the  ear  in  making  this  separation.  On  the  otlier  liand,  a 
musical  tone  strikes   the  ear  as  a  perfectly  undisturbed,  uniform  sound  which 

Digitized  by  V^OOQIC 


8    »  NOISE   AND  MUSICAL  TONE.  part  i. 

rRmains  unaltered  as  long  as  it  exists,  and  it  presents  no  alternation  of  various 
kinds  of  constituents.  To  this  then  corresponds  a  simple,  regular  kind  of  sensation, 
whereas  in  a  noise  many  various  sensations  of  musical  tone  are  irregularly  mixed 
up  and  as  it  were  tumbled  about  in  confusion.  We  can  easily  compound  noises 
out  of  musical  tones,  as,  for  example,  by  simultaneously  striking  all  the  keys  con- 
tained in  one  or  two  octaves  of  a  pianoforte.  This  shews  us  that  musical  tones 
are  the  simpler  and  more  regular  elements  of  the  sensations  of  hearing,  and  that 
we  have  consequently  first  to  study  the  laws  and  peculiarities  of  this  class  of 
sensations. 

Then  comes  the  further  question  :  On  what  difference  in  the  external  means  of 
excitement  does  the  difference  between  noise  and  musical  tone  depend  ?  The 
normal  and  usual  means  of  excitement  for  the  human  ear  is  atmospheric  vibration. 
The  irregularly  alternating  sensation  of  the  ear  in  the  case  of  noises  leads  us  to 

^  conclude  that  for  these  the  vibration  of  the  air  must  also  change  irregularly.  For 
musical  tones  on  the  other  hand  we  anticipate  a  regular  motion  of  the  air,  con- 
tinuing uniformly,  and  in  its  turn  excited  by  an  equally  regular  motion  of  the 
sonorous  body,  whose  impulses  were  conducted  to  the  ear  by  the  air. 

Those  regular  motions  which  produce  musical  tones  have  been  exactly  investi- 
gated by  physicists.  They  are  oscillations,  vibrations,  or  swings,  that  is,  up  and 
down,  or  to  and  fro  motions  of  sonorous  bodies,  and  it  is  necessary  that  these 
oscillations  should  be  regularly  periodic.  By  a  periodic  motion  we  mean  one  which 
constantly  returns  to  the  same  condition  after  exactly  equal  intervals  of  time.  The 
ength  of  the  equal  intervals  of  time  between  one  state  of  the  motion  and  its  next 
exact  repetition,  we  call  the  length  of  the  oscillation  vibration  or  swing,  or  the 
period  of  the  motion.  In  what  manner  the  moving  body  actually  moves  during 
one  period,  is  perfectly  indifferent.  As  illustrations  of  periodical  motion,  take  the 
motion  of  a  clock  pendulum,  of  a  stone  attached  to  a  string  and  whirled  round  in 

^  a  circle  with  uniform  velocity,  of  a  hammer  made  to  rise  and  fall  uniformly  by  its 
connection  with  a  water  wheel.  All  these  motions,  however  different  be  their 
form,  are  periodic  in  the  sense  here  explained.  The  length  of  their  periods,  which 
in  the  cases  adduced  is  generally  from  one  to  several  seconds,  is  relatively  long  in 
comparison  with  the  much  shorter  periods  of  the  vibrations  producing  musical 
tones,  the  lowest  or  deepest  of  which  makes  at  least  30  in  a  second,  while  in  other 
cases  their  number  may  increase  to  several  thousand  in  a  second. 

Our  definition  of  periodic  motion  then  enables  us  to  answer  the  question  pro- 
posed as  follows  :-iT/i6  sensation  of  a  musical  tone  is  due  to  a  rapid  periodic 
motion  of  the  sonorous  body  ;  tJie  sensation  of  a  noise  to  non-periodic  motions. 

The  musical  vibrations  of  solid  bodies  are  often  visible.  Although  they  may 
be  too  rapid  for  the  eye  to  follow  them  singly,  we  easily  recognise  that  a  sounding 
string,  or  tuning-fork,  or  the  tongue  of  a  reed-pipe,  is  rapidly  vibrating  between  two 
fixed  limits,  and  the  regular,  apparently  immovable  image  that  we  see,  notwith- 

^  Ktanding  the  real  motion  of  the  body,  leads  us  to  conclude  that  the  backward  and 
forward  motions  are  quite  regular.  In  other  cases  we  can  feel  the  swinging  motions 
of  sonorous  sohds.  Thus,  the  player  feels  the  trembling  of  the  reed  in  the  mouth- 
piece of  a  clarinet,  oboe,  or  bassoon,  or  of  his  own  lips  in  the  mouthpieces  of 
trumpets  and  trombones. 

The  motions  proceeding  from  the  sounding  bodies  are  usually  conducted  to  our 
ear  by  means  of  the  atmosphere.  The  particles  of  air  must  also  execute  periodi- 
cally recurrent  vibrations,  in  order  to  excite  the  sensation  of  a  musical  tone  m  our 
ear.  This  is  actually  the  case,  although  in  daily  experience  sound  at  first  seems 
to  be  some  agent,  which  is  constantly  advancing  through  the  air,  and  propagating 
itself  further  and  further.  We  must,  however,  here  distinguish  between  the  motion 
of  the  individual  particles  of  air — which  takes  place  periodically  backwards  and 
forwards  within  very  narrow  hmits — and  the  propagation  of  the  sonorous  tremor. 
The  latter  is  constantly  advancing  by  the  constant  attraction  of  fresh  particles  into 
its  spiicre  of  tremor. 

Digitized  by  VjOOQIC 


CHAP.  1.  PROPAGATION  OP  SOUND.  9 

This  is  a  peculiarity  of  all  so-called  undulatory  moHons.  Suppose  a  stone  to 
be  thrown  into  a  piece  of  calm  water.  Bound  the  spot  struck  there  forms  a  little 
ring  of  wave,  which,  advancing  equally  in  all  directions,  expands  to  a  constantly 
increasing  circle.  Corresponding  to  this  ring  of  wave,  soimd  also  proceeds  in  the 
air  from  the  excited  point  and  advances  in  all  directions  as  far  as  the  limits  of  the 
mass  of  air  extend.  The  process  in  the  air  is  essentially  identical  with  that  on  the 
surface  of  the  water.  The  principal  difference  consists  in  the  spherical  propagation 
of  sound  in  all  directions  through  the  atmosphere  which  fills  all  surrounding  space, 
whereas  the  waves  of  the  water  can  only  advance  in  rings  or  circles  on  its  surface. 
The  crests  of  the  waves  of  water  correspond  in  the  waves  of  sound  to  spherical 
shells  where  the  air  is  condensed,  and  the  troughs  to  shells  where  it  is  rarefied. 
On  the  free  surface  of  the  water,  the  mass  when  compressed  can  slip  upwards  and 
so  form  ridges,  hut  in  the  interior  of  the  sea  of  air,  the  mass  must  be  condensed, 
as  there  is  no  unoccupied  spot  for  its  escape.  II 

The  waves  of  water,  therefore,  continually  advance  without  returning.  But 
we  must  not  suppose  that  the  particles  of  water  of  which  the  waves  are  composed 
advance  in  a  similar  manner  to  the  waves  themselves.  The  motion  of  the  particles 
of  water  on  the  surface  can  easily  be  rendered  visible  by  floating  a  chip  of  wood 
upon  it.  This  will  exactly  share  the  motion  of  the  adjacent  particles.  Now,  such 
a  chip  is  not  carried  on  by  the  rings  of  wave.  It  only  bobs  up  and  down  and 
finally  rests  on  its  original  spot.  The  adjacent  particles  of  water  move  in  the  same 
manner.  When  the  ring  of  wave  reaches  them  they  are  set  bobbing ;  when  it  has 
passed  over  them  they  are  still  in  their  old  place,  and  remain  there  at  rest,  while 
the  ring  of  wave  continues  to  advance  towards  fresh  spots  on  the  surface  of  the 
water,  and  sets  new  particles  of  water  in  motion.  Hence  the  waves  which  pass 
over  the  surface  of  the  water  are  constantly  built  up  of  fresh  particles  of  water. 
What  really  advances  as  a  wave  is  only  the  tremor,  the  altered  form  of  the  surface, 
while  the  individual  particles  of  water  themselves  merely  move  up  and  down  % 
transiently,  and  never  depart  far  from  their  original  position. 

The  same  relation  is  seen  still  more  clearly  in  the  waves  of  a  rope  or  chain. 
Take  a  flexible  string  of  several  feet  in  length,  or  a  thin  metal  chain,  hold  it  at  one 
end  and  let  the  other  hang  down,  stretched  by  its  own  weight  alone.  Now,  move 
the  hand  by  which  you  hold  it  quickly  to  one  side  and  back  again.  The  excursion 
which  we  have  caused  in  the  upper  end  of  the  string  by  moving  the  hand,  will  run 
down  it  as  a  kind  of  wave,  so  that  constantly  lower  parts  of  the  string  will  make  a 
sidewards  excursion  while  the  upper  return  again  into  the  straight  position  of  rest. 
But  it  is  evident  that  while  the  wave  runs  down,  each  individual  particle  of  the 
string  can  have  only  moved  horizontally  backwards  and  forwards,  and  can  have 
taken  no  share  at  all  in  the  advance  of  the  wave. 

The  experiment  succeeds  still  better  with  a  long  elastic  line,  such  as  a  thick 
piece  of  india-rubber  tubing,  or  a  brass- wire  spiral  spring,  from  eight  to  twelve  feet 
in  length,  fastened  at  one  end,  and  slightly  stretched  by  being  held  with  the  hand  «| 
at  the  other.  The  hand  is  then  easily  able  to  excite  waves  which  will  run  very 
regularly  to  the  other  end  of  the  line,  be  there  reflected  and  return.  In  this  case 
it  is  also  evident  that  it  can  be  no  part  of  the  line  itself  which  runs  backwards  and 
forwards,  but  that  the  advancing  wave  is  composed  of  continually  fresh  particles 
of  the  line.  By  these  examples  the  reader  will  be  able  to  form  a  mental  image  of 
the  kind  of  motion  to  which  sound  belongs,  where  the  material  particles  of  the 
body  merely  make  periodical  oscillations,  while  the  tremor  itself  is  constantly 
propagated  forwards. 

Now  let  us  return  to  the  surface  of  the  water.  We  have  supposed  that  one  of 
its  points  has  been  struck  by  a  stone  and  set  in  motion.  This  motion  has  spread 
out  in  the  form  of  a  ring  of  wave  over  the  surface  of  the  water,  and  having  reached 
the  cliip  of  wood  has  set  it  bobbing  up  and  down.  Hence  by  means  of  the  wave, 
the  motion  which  the  stone  first  excited  in  one  point  of  the  surface  of  the  water 
has  been  communicated  to  the  chip  which  was  at  another  point  bf  the  same  surface. 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQ IC 


lo  FORCE,  PITCH,   AND  QUALITY.  part  i. 

The  process  which  goes  on  in  the  atmospheric  ocean  about  us,  is  of  a  precisely 
similar  nature.  For  the  stone  substitute  a  sounding  body,  which  shakes  the  air  ; 
for  the  chip  of  wood  substitute  the  human  ear,  on  which  impinge  the  waves  of  air 
excited  by  the  shock,  setting  its  movable  parts  in  vibration.  The  waves  of  air 
proceeding  from  a  sounding  body,  transport  the  tremor  to  the  human  ear  exactly 
in  the  same  way  as  the  water  transports  the  tremor  produced  by  the  stone  to  the 
floating  chip. 

In  this  way  also  it  is  easy  to  see  how  a  body  which  itself  makes  periodical 
oscillations,  will  necessarily  set  the  particles  of  air  in  periodical  motion.  A  falling 
stone  gives  the  surface  of  the  water  a  single  shock.  Now  replace  the  stone  by  a 
regular  series  of  drops  falling  from  a  vessel  with  a  smaU  orifice.  Every  separate 
drop  will  excite  a  ring  of  wave,  each  ring  of  wave  will  advance  over  the  surface  of 
the  water  precisely  like  its  predecessor,  and  will  be  in  the  same  way  followed  by 

V  its  successors.  In  this  manner  a  regular  series  of  concentric  rings  wiU  be  formed 
and  propagated  over  the  surface  of  the  water.  The  number  of  drops  which  fall 
into  the  water  in  a  second  will  be  the  number  of  waves  which  reach  our  floating 
chip  in  a  second,  and  the  number  of  times  that  this  chip  will  therefore  bob  up  and 
down  in  a  second,  thus  executing  a  periodical  motion,  the  period  of  which  is  equal 
to  the  interval  of  time  between  the  falling  of  consecutive  drops.  In  the  same  way 
for  the  atmosphere,  a  periodically  oscillating  sonorous  body  produces  a  similar 
periodical  motion,  first  in  the  mass  of  air,  and  then  in  the  drumskin  of  our  ear, 
and  the  period  of  these  vibrations  must  be  the  same  as  that  of  the  vibration  in  the 
sonorous  body. 

Having  thus  spoken  of  the  principal  division  of  sound  into  Noise  and  Musical 
Tones,  and  then  described  the  general  motion  of  the  air  for  these  tones,  we  pass 
on  to  the  peculiarities  which  distinguish  such  tones  one  from  the  other.  We  are 
acquainted  with  three  points  of  difference  in  musical  tones,  confining  our  attention 

H  in  the  first  place  to  such  tones  as  are  isolatedly  produced  by  our  usual  musical 
instruments,  and  excluding  the  simultaneous  sounding  of  the  tones  of  difi'erent 
instruments.     Musical  tones  are  distinguished  : — 

1.  By  their  force ^ 

2.  By  their  ^Jtte^, 

3.  By  their  qiiality. 

It  is  unnecessary  to  explain  what  we  mean  by  the  force  and  pitch  of  a  tone. 
By  the  quality  of  a  tone  we  mean  that  peculiarity  which  distinguishes  the  musical 
tone  of  a  violin  from  that  of  a  flute  or  that  of  a  clarinet,  or  thafc  of  the  human 
voice,  when  all  these  instruments  produce  the  same  note  at  the  same  pitch. 

We  have  now  to  explain  what  peculiarities  of  the  motion  of  sound  correspond 
to  these  three  principal  differences  between  musical  tones. 

First,  We  easily  recognise  that  the  force  of  a  musical  tone  increases  and  dimi- 
nishes with  the  extent  or  so-caUed  amjditude  of  the  oscillations  of  the  particles  of 
H  the  sounding  body.  When  we  strike  a  string,  its  vibrations  are  at  first  sufficiently 
large  for  us  to  see  them,  and  its  corresponding  tone  is  loudest.  The  visible 
vibrations  become  smaller  and  smaller,  and  at  the  same  time  the  loudness 
diminishes.  The  same  observation  can  be  made  on  strings  excited  by  a  violin 
bow,  and  on  the  reeds  of  reed-pipes,  and  on  many  other  sonorous  bodies.  The 
same  conclusion  results  from  the  diminution  of  the  loudness  of  a  tone  when  we 
increase  our  distance  from  the  sounding  body  in  the  open  air,  although  the  pitch 
and  quality  remain  unaltered ;  for  it  is  only  the  amplitude  of  tlie  oscillations  of 
the  particles  of  air  which  diminishes  as  their  distance  from  the  sounding  body 
increases.  Hence  loudness  must  depend  on  this  amphtude,  and  none  other  of  the 
properties  of  sound  do  so.* 

*  Mechanically  the  force  of  the  oscillations  no  measure  can  be  found  for  the  intensity  of 

for  tones  of  different  pitch  is  measured   by  the  sensation  of  sound,  that  is,  for  the  loudness 

their  vis  viva^  that  is,  by  the  square  of  the  of  sound,  which  will  hold  all  pitches.     [See 

greatest  velocity   attained   by  the   oscillating  the  addition  to  a  footnote  on  p.  75^^,  referring 

particles.     But  the  ear  has  different  degrees  of  especially  to  this  passage.   -  TraaaUitor.] 
sensibility  for  tones  of  different  pitch,  so  that  Digitized  by  V^jOOQIC 


CHAP.  I. 


PITCH  AND  THE   SIEEN. 


II 


The  second  essential  difference  between  different  musical  tones  consists  in 
their  pitch.  Daily  experience  shews  us  that  musical  tones  of  the  same  pitch  can 
be  produced  upon  most  diverse  instruments  by  means  of  most  diverse  mechanical 
contrivances,  and  with  most  diverse  degrees  of  loudness.  All  the  motions  of  the 
air  thus  excited  must  be  periodic,  because  they  would  not  otherwise  excite  in  us 
the  sensation  of  a  musical  tone.  But  the  sort  of  motion  within  each  single 
period  may  be  any  whatever,  and  yet  if  the  length  of  the  periodic  time  of  two 
musical  tones  is  the  same,  they  have  the  same  pitch.  Hence  :  Pitch  depends 
solely  an  the  length  of  time  in  which  each  single  inhration  is  executed,  or,  which 
comes  to  the  same  thing,  on  the  number  of  vibrations  completed  in  a  given  time. 
We  are  accustomed  to  take  a  second  as  the  unit  of  time,  and  shall  consequently 
mean  by  the  pitch  number  [or  frequency]  of  a  tone,  the  number  of  vibrations  which 
the  particles  of  a  sounding  body  perform  in  one  second  of  time.*  It  is  self-evident 
that  we  find  the  periodic  time  or  vibrational  period,  that  is  length  of  time  which  ^ 
is  occupied  in  performing  a  single  vibration  backwards  and  forwards,  by  dividing 
one  second  of  time  by  the  pitch  number. 

Musical  tones  are  said  to  be  higher,  the  greater-  their  pitch  numbers,  that  is, 
the  shorter  their  vibrational  periods. 

The  exact  determination  of  the  pitch  number  for  such  elastic  bodies  as  produce 
andible  tones,  presents  considerable  diflBculty,  and  physicists  had  to  contrive  many 
comparatively  complicated  processes  in  order  to  solve  this  problem  for  each 
particular  case.  Mathematical  theory  and  numerous  experiments  had  to  render 
matual  assistance.f  It  is  consequently  very  convenient  for  the  demonstration  of 
the  fundamental  facts  in  this  department  of  knowledge,  to  be  able  to  apply  a 
peculiar  instrument  for  producing  musical  tones — the  so-called  siren — which  is 
constructed  in  such  a  manner  as  to  determine  the  pitch  number  of  the  tone 
produced,  by  a  direct  observation.  The  principal  parts  of  the  simplest  form  of 
the  siren  are  shewn  in  fig.  i ,  after  Seebeck.  ^ 

A  is  a  thin  disc  of  cardboard  or  tinplate,  which  can  be  set  in  rapid  rotation 
about  its  axle  b  by  means  of  a  string  f  f,  which  passes  over  a  larger  wheel.  On 
the  margin  of  the  disc  there  is  punched  a  set  of  holes  at  equal  intervals  :  of  these 

there  are  twelve  in  the  figure ;  one  or 
more  similar  series  of  holes  at  equal 
distances  are  introduced  on  concentric 
circles,  (there  is  one  such  of  eight  holes 
in  the  figure),  c  is  a  pipe  which  is 
.directed  over  one  of  the  holes.  Now, 
'  on  setting  the  disc  in  rotation  and  blow- 
ing through  the  pipe  c,  the  air  will  pass 
freeely  whenever  one  of  the  holes  comes 
under  the  end  of  the  pipe,  but  will  be 
checked  whenever  an  unpierced  portion  m 
of  tlie  disc  comes  under  it.  Each  hole 
of  the  disc,  then,  that  passes  the  end  of  the  pipe  lets  a  single  puff  of  air  escape. 
Supposing  the  disc  to  make  a  single  revolution  and  the  pipe  to  be  directed  to  the 


Pig.  I. 


♦  IThe pitch  number  yf&s  called  the  •vibra- 
tional namber  *  in  the  first  edition  of  this  trans- 
lation. The  pitch  number  ol  a  note  is  commonly 
called  the  pilch  of  the  note.  By  a  convenient 
abbreviation  we  often  write  a'  440,  meaning 
the  note  a'  having  the  pitch  number  440 ;  or 
say  that  the  pitch  of  a'  is  440  vib.  that  is,  440 
doable  vibrations  in  a  second.  The  second 
^^^tm  frequency f  which  I  have  introduced  into 
the  text,  as  it  is  much  used  by  acousticians, 
properly  represents  Dhe  number  of  times  tliat 
•»y  peruxUcully  recurring  event  liappens  in 
otic  iuo7td  of  titne^  and,  applied  to  double 
vibrations,  it  means  the  same  a;»  pitch  number. 


The  pitch  of  a  musical  instrument  is  the  pitch 
of  the  note  by  which  it  is  tuned.  But  as  pitch 
is  properly  a  sensation,  it  is  necessary  here 
to  distinguish  from  this  sensation  the  pitch 
number  or  frequency  of  vibration  by  which  it 
is  measured.  The  larger  the  pitch  number, 
the  higher  or  sharper  the  pitch  is  said  to  be. 
The  lower  the  pitch  number  the  deeper  or 
flatter  the  pitch.  These  arc  all  metaphorical 
expressions  which  must  be  taken  strictly  in 
this  sense. — Translator.] 

f  [An  account  of  the  more  exact  modern 
methods  is  given  in  App.  XX.  sect.  B. — 
Translator.] 

Digitized  by  V^.OOQIC 


13 


PITCH  AND  THE   SIREN. 


PART    I. 


outer  circle  of  holes,  we  have  twelve  puffs  corresponding  to  the  twelve  holes ;  but 
if  the  pipe  is  directed  to  the  inner  circle  we  have  only  eight  puffs.  If  the  disc  is 
made  to  revolve  ten  times  in  one  second,  the  outer  circle  would  produce  1 20  puffs 
in  one  second,  which  would  give  rise  to  a  weak  and  deep  musical  tone,  and  the 
inner  circle  eighty  puffs.  Generally,  if  we  know  the  number  of  revolutions  which 
the  disc  makes  in  a  second,  and  the  number  of  holes  in  the  series  to  which  the 
tube  is  directed,  the  product  of  these  two  numbers  evidently  gives  the  number  of 
puffs  in  a  second.  This  number  is  consequently  far  easier  to  determine  exactly 
than  in  any  other  musical  instrument,  and  sirens  are  accordingly  extremely  well 
adapted  for  studying  all  changes  in  musical  tones  resulting  from  the  alterations 
and  ratios  of  the  pitch  numbers. 

The  fonn  of  siren  here  described  gives  only  a  weak  tone.     I  have  placed  it  first 
because  its  action  can  be  most  readily  understood,  and,  by  changing  the  disc,  it 


Pm. 


Fig.  3. 


Fig.  4. 


can  be  easily  applied  to  experiments  of  very  different  descriptions.  A  stronger  tone 
is  produced  in  the  siren  of  Cagniard  de  la  Tour,  shewn  in  figures  2,  3,  and  4,  above. 
Here  s  s  is  the  rotating  disc,  of  which  the  upper  surface  is  shewn  in  fig.  3,  and 
the  side  is  seen  in  figs.  2  and  4.  It  is  placed  over  a  windchest  A  A,  which  is 
connected  with  a  bellows  by  the  pipe  B  B.  The  cover  of  the  windchest  A  A, 
which  lies  immediately  under  the  rotating  disc  s  s,  is  pierced  with  precisely  the 
same  number  of  holes  as  the  disc,  and  the  direction  of  the  holes  pierced  in  the 
cover  of  the  chest  is  obhque  to  that  of  the  holes  in  the  disc,  as  shewn  in  fig.  4, 
which  is  a  vertical  section  of  the  instrument  through  the  line  n  n  in  fig.  3.  This 
position  of  the  holes  enables  the  wind  escaping  from  A  A  to  set  the  disc  s  s  in 
rotation,  and  by  increasing  the  pressure  of  tlie  bellows,  as  much  as  50  or  60 
rotations  in  a  second  can  be  produced.  Since  all  the  holes  of  one  circle  are  blown 
through  at  the  same  time  in  this  siren,  a  mucli  more  powerful  tone  is  produced 
than  in   Seeheck's,  fig.  i   (p.  iic).     To  record  tlie  revolutions,  a  counter  z  z  is 

Digitized  by  V^ O OQ IC 


CHAP.  I.  PITCH  AND  INTERVAL.  13 

introdaced,  connected  with  a  toothed  wheel  which  works  in  the  screw  t,  and 
advances  one  tooth  for  each  revolution  of  the  disc  s  s.  By  the  handle  h  this 
counter  may  be  moved  slightly  to  one  side,  so  that  the  wheelwork  and  screw  may 
be  connected  or  disconnected  at  pleasure.  If  they  are  connected  at  the  beginning 
of  one  second,  and  disconnected  at  the  beginning  of  another,  the  hand  of  the 
counter  shows  how  many  revolutions  of  the  disc  have  been  made  in  the  corre- 
sponding number  of  seconds.* 

Dove  t  introduced  into  this  siren  several  rows  of  holes  through  which  the  wind 
might  be  directed,  or  from  which  it  might  be  cut  off,  at  pleasure.  A  polyphonic 
siren  of  this  description  with  other  peculiar  arrangements  will  be  figured  and 
described  in  Chapter  VIII.,  fig.  56. 

It  is  clear  that  when  the  pierced  disc  of  one  of  these  sirens  is  made  to  revolve 
with  a  uniform  velocity,  and  the  air  escapes  through  the  holes  in  puffs,  the  motion 
of  the  air  thus  produced  must  be  periodic  in  the  sense  already  explained.  The  If 
holes  stand  at  equal  intervals  of  space,  and  hence  on  rotation  foUow  each  other  at 
equal  intervals  of  time.  Through  every  hole  there  is  poured,  as  it  were,  a  drop  of 
air  into  the  external  atmospheric  ocean,  exciting  waves  in  it,  which  succeed  each 
other  at  uniform  intervals  of  time,  just  as  was  the  case  when  regularly  falling 
drops  impinged  upon  a  surface  of  water  (p.  loa).  Within  each  separate  period, 
each  individual  puff  will  have  considerable  variations  of  form  in  sirens  of  different 
construction,  depending  on  the  different  diameters  of  the  holes,  their  distance  from 
each  other,  and  the  shape  of  the  extremity  of  the  pipe  which  conveys  the  air  ;  but 
m  every  case,  as  long  as  the  velocity  of  rotation  and  the  position  of  the  pipe  remain 
unaltered,  a  regularly  periodic  motion  of  the  air  must  result,  and  consequently  the 
sensation  of  a  musical  tone  must  be  excited  in  the  ear,  and  this  is  actually  the 
case. 

It  results  immediately  from  experiments  with  the  siren  that  two  series  of  the 
same  number  of  holes  revolving  with  the  same  velocity,  give  musical  tones  of  the  f 
same  pitch,  quite  independently  of  the  size  and  form  of  the  holes,  or  of  the  pipe. 
We  even  obtain  a  musical  tone  of  the  same  pitch  if  we  allow  a  metal  point  to 
strike  in  the  holes  as  they  revolve  instead  of  blowing.  Hence  it  follows  firstly  that 
the  pitch  of  a  tone  depends  only  on  the  number  of  puffs  or  swings,  and  not  on 
their  form,  force,  or  method  of  production.  Further  it  is  very  easily  seen  witli 
this  instrument  that  on  increasing  the  velocity  of  rotation  and  consequently  the 
number  of  puffs  produced  in  a  second,  the  pitch  becomes  sharper  or  higher.  The 
same  result  ensues  if,  maintaining  a  uniform  velocity  of  rotation,  we  first  blow  into 
a  series  with  a  smaller  and  then  into  a  series  with  a  greater  number  of  holes. 
The  latter  gives  the  sharper  or  higher  pitch. 

With  the  same  instrument  we  also  very  easily  find  the  remarkable  relation 
which  the  pitch  numbers  of  two  musical  tones  must  possess  in  order  to  form  a 
consonant  interval.  Take  a  series  of  8  and  anotlier  of  16  holes  on  a  disc,  and 
blow  into  both  sets  while  the  disc  is  kept  at  uniform  velocity  of  rotation.  Two  % 
tones  will  be  heard  which  stand  to  one  another  in  the  exact  relation  of  an  Octave. 
Increase  the  velocity  of  rotation ;  both  tones  will  become  sharper,  but  both  will 
continue  at  the  new  pitch  to  form  the  interval  of  an  Octave. J  '\  Hence  we  conclude 
that  a  mtisical  tone  which  is  an  Octave  higher  than  another,  makes  exactly  twice 
as  many  vibrations  in  a  given  time  as  the  latter. 

*  See  Appendix  I.  names  of  all  the  intervals  usually  distinguished 

t   [Pronounce  Doli-veh,  in  two  syllables.—  are  also  given  in  App.  XX.  sect.  D.,  with  the 

Translator.]  corresponding  ratios  and  cents.     These  names 

t   [When  two  notes  have  different  pitch  were  in  the  first  place  derived  from  the  ordinal 

numbers,  there    is  said    to    be  an    interval  number  of  the  note  in  the  scales,  or  succes- 

bctween  them.    This  gives  rise  to  a  sensa-  sions  of  continually  sharper  notes.  The  Octave 

tion,  very  differently  appreciated  by  different  is  the  eighth  note  in  the  major  scale.  An  octave 

individuals,  but   in  all   cases  the  interval  is  is  a  set  of  notes  lying  within  an  Octave.     Ob- 

measurcd  by  the  ratio  of  the  pitch  number s^  serve  that  in  this  translation  aU  names  of  in- 

and,  for  some  purposes,  more  conveniently  by  tervals  commence  with  a  capital  letter,   to 

other  numbers  called  cents ^  derived  from  these  prevent  ambiguity,  as  almost  all  such  words 

ratios,  as  explained  in  App.  XX.  sect.  C.    The  are  also  used  in  other  senses. — Translator,'] 


Digitized  by  V^jOOQ  l€ 


14  PITCH  AND  INTEEVAL.  pakt  i. 

The  disc  shewn  in  fig.  i,  p.  iic,  has  two  circles  of  8  and  12  holes  respectively. 
Each,  blown  successively,  gives  two  tones  which  form  with  each  other  a  perfect 
Fifth,  independently  of  the  velocity  of  rotation  of  the  disc.  Hence,  two  musical 
tones  stand  in  the  relation  of  a  so-called  Fifth  when  the  higJier  tone  makes  three 
vibrations  in  the  same  time  as  the  lower  makes  two. 

If  we  obtain  a  muFdcal  tone  by  blowing  into  a  circle  of  8  holes,  we  require  a 
circle  of  16  holes  for  its  Octave,  and  12  for  its  Fifth.  Hence  the  ratio  of  the 
pitch  numbers  of  the  Fifth  and  the  Octave  is  12  :  16  or  3  :  4.  But  the  interval 
between  the  Fifth  and  the  Octave  is  the  Fourth,  so  that  we  see  that  when  two 
musical  tones  form  a  Fourth,  the  higher  makes  four  vibrations  while  the  lower 
tnakes  three. 

The  polyphonic  siren  of  Dove  has  usually  four  circles  of  8,  10,  12  and  16  holes 
respectively.  The  series  of  16  holes  gives  the  Octave  of  the  series  of  8  holes,  and 
1[  the  Fourth  of  the  series  of  1 2  holes.  The  series  of  1 2  holes  gives  the  Fifth  of  the 
series  of  8  holes,  and  the  minor  Third  of  the  series  of  10  holes.  While  the  series  of 
10  holes  gives  the  major  Third  of  the  series  of  8  holes.  The  four  series  con- 
sequently give  the  constituent  musical  tones  of  a  major  chord. 

By  these  and  similar  experiments  we  find  the  following  relations  of  the  pitch 
numbers : — 

1  :    2     Octave 

2  :   3    Fifth 

3  :   4    Fourth 

4  :   5     major  Third 

5  :   6     minor  Third 

When  the  fundamental  tone  of  a  given  interval  is  taken  an  Octave  higher,  the 
interval  is  said  to  be  inverted.    Thus  a  Fourth  is  an  inverted  Fifth,  a  minor  Sixth 
f  an  inverted  major  Third,  and  a  major  Sixth  an  inverted  minor  Third.     The  corre- 
sponding ratios  of  the  pitch  numbers  are  consequently  obtained  by  doubling  the 
smaller  number  in  the  original  interval. 

From  2  :  3     the  Fifth,  we  thus  have       3  :  4    the  Fourth 
„     4:5     the  major  Third      ...      5  :  8     the  minor  Sixth 
„     5:6     the  minor  Third,     6  :  10=3  :  5     the  major  Sixth. 

These  are  all  the  consonant  intervals  which  lie  within  the  compass  of  an 
Octave.  With  the  exception  of  the  minor  Sixth,  which  is  really  the  most  imperfect 
of  the  above  consonances,  the  ratios  of  their  vibrational  numbers  are  all  expressed 
by  means  of  the  whole  numbers,  i,  2,  3,  4,  5,  6. 

Comparatively  simple  and  easy  experiments  with  the  siren,  therefore,  corrobo- 
rate that  remarkable  law  mentioned  in  the  Introduction  (p.  id),  according  to  which 
the  pitch  numbers  of  consonant  musical  tones  bear  to  each  other  ratios  expressible 
If  by  small  whole  numbers.  In  the  course  of  our  investigation  we  shall  employ  the 
same  instrument  to  verify  more  completely  the  strictness  and  exactness  of  this 
law. 

Long  before  anything  was  known  of  pitch  numbers,  or  the  means  of  countmg 
them,  Pythagoras  had  discovered  that  if  a  string  be  divided  into  two  parts  by  a 
bridge,  in  such  a  way  as  to  give  two  consonant  musical  tones  when  struck,  the 
lengths  of  these  parts  must  be  in  the  ratio  of  these  whole  numbers.  If  the  bridge 
is  so  placed  that  f  of  the  string  lie  to  the  right,  and  ^  on  the  left,  so  that  the  two 
lengths  are  in  the  ratio  of  2  :  i,  they  produce  the  interval  of  an  Octave,  the  greater 
length  giving  the  deeper  tone.  Placing  the  bridge  so  that  f  of  the  string  lie  on 
the  right  and  f  on  the  left,  the  ratio  of  the  two  lengths  is  3:2,  and  the  interval 
is  a  Fifth. 

These  measurements  had  been  executed  with  great  precision  by  the  Greek 
musicians,  and  had  given  rise  to  a  system  of  tones,  contrived  with  considerable 
art.    For  these  measurements  they  used  a  peculiar  instrument,  the  motiochord. 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


CHAP.  I.  PITCH  NUMBERS  IN   JUST  MAJOR  SCALE.  15 

consisting  of  a  sounding  board  and  box  on  which  a  single  string  was  stretched 
with  a  scale  below,  so  as  to  set  the  bridge  correctly.* 

It  was  not  till  much  later  that,  through  the  investigations  of  Galileo  (1638), 
Newton,  Euler  (1729),  and  Daniel  Bemouilli  (1771),  the  law  governing  the 
motions  of  strings  became  known,  and  it  was  thus  found  that  the  simple  ratios  of 
the  lengths  of  the  strings  existed  also  for  the  pitch  numbers  of  thp  tones  they  pro- 
duced, and  that  they  consequently  belonged  to  the  musical  intervals  of  the  tones 
of  all  instruments,  and  were  not  confined  to  the  lengths  of  strings  through  which 
the  law  had  been  first  discovered. 

This  relation  of  whole  numbers  to  musical  consonances  was  from  all  time 
looked  upon  as  a  wonderful  mystery  of  deep  significance.  The  Pythagoreans 
themselves  made  use  of  it  in  their  speculations  on  the  harmony  of  the  spheres. 
From  that  time  it  remained  partly  the  goal  and  partly  the  starting  point  of  the 
strangest  and  most  venturesome,  fajitastic  or  philosophic  combinations,  till  in  ^ 
modem  times  the  majority  of  investigators  adopted  the  notion  accepted  by  Euler 
himself,  that  the  human  mind  had  a  peculiar  pleasure  in  simple  ratios,  because  it 
could  better  understand  them  and  comprehend  their  bearings.  But  it  remained 
uninvestigated  how  the  mind  of  a  listener  not  versed  in  physics,  who  perhaps  was 
not  even  aware  that  musical  tones  depended  on  periodical  vibrations,  contrived  to 
recognise  and  compare  these  ratios  of  the  pitch  numbers.  To  shew  what  pro- 
cesses taking  place  in  the  ear,  render  sensible  the  difference  between  consonance 
and  dissonance,  will  be  one  of  the  principal  problems  in  the  second  part  of  this 
work. 

Calculation  of  the  Pitch  Numbebs  fob  all  the  Tones  of  the 

Musical  Scale. 

By  means  of  the  ratios  of  the  pitch  numbers  already  assigned  for  the  consonant 
intervals,  it  is  easy,  by  pursuing  these  intervals  throughout,  to  calculate  the  ratios  f 
for  the  whole  extent  of  the  musical  scale. 

The  major  triad  or  chord  of  three  tones,  consists  of  a  major  Third  and  a  Fifth, 
Hence  its  ratios  are : 

C:E:  G 
I  :  f  :  ^ 
or    4:5:6 

If  we  associate  with  this  triad  that  of  its  dominant  G  :  B  :  Dy  and  that  of  its 
sub- dominant  F  :  A  :  C,  each  of  which  has  one  tone  in  common  with  the  triad  of 
the  tonic  C  :  E  :  G,  we  obtain  the  complete  series  of  tones  for  the  major  scale  of 
C,  with  the  following  ratios  of  the  pitch  numbers : 

C  :  D  :  E  :  F  :  G  :  A  :  B  :c 

T*9-fi     •4-3     •5-15.9 

[or    24  :  27  :  30  :  32  :  36  :  40  :  45  :  48] 

In  order  to  extend  the  calculation  to  other  octaves,  we  shall  adopt  the  following 
notation  of  musical  tones,  marking  the  higher  octaves  by  accents,  as  is  usual  in 
Germany ,t  as  follows : 

I.  The  unaccented  or  small  octave  (the  4-foot  octave  on  the  organj ): — 


m 


zzso 


rz2i 


c          d          e  f          g          a          b 

*    r Ab  the  monochord  is  very  liable  to  error,  below  the  letters,  which  are  typographically 

these  results  were  happy  generalisations  from  inconvenient.    Hence  the  German  notation  is 

necessarily    imperfect    experiments.— Tratw-  retained. — Translator.] 

lator.]  J   [The  note  C  in  the  small  octave  was 

t   [English  works  use  strokes  above  and  once  emitted  by  an  organ  pipe  4  feet  in  length : 

Digitized  by  V^jOOSlC 


i6  PITCH  NUMBERS  IN  JUST  MAJOR  SCALE. 

2.  The  oncC'Ctccented  octave  (2-foot) : — 


PABT  I. 


i 


..^ 


ra" 


^       c'         ci'         e'         / 
3.  The  twice-accented  octave  (i-foot) : — 


-^ 


1221 


nr22i 


(Z" 


/' 


r 


6" 


And  so  on  for  higher  octaves.     Below  the  small  octave  lies  the  great  octave, 
written  with  unaccented  capital  letters  ;  its  G  requires  an  organ  pipe  of  eight  feet 
^  in  length,  and  hence  it  is  called  the  8-foot  octave. 

4.  Great  or  Z-foot  octa/ve  : — 


D 


E 


F 


O 


A 


Below  this  follows  the  16-foot  or  contra-octave  ;  the  lowest  on  the  pianoforte 
and  most  organs,  the  tones  of  which  may  be  represented  by  C^  D,  E,  F^  G,  A,  B^, 
with  an  inverted  accent.  On  great  organs  there  is  a  still  deeper,  32 -foot  octave,  tlie 
tones  of  which  may  be  written  C,,  Dj,  E„  F,,  G,,  A,,  B^„  with  two  inverted  accents, 
but  they  scarcely  retain  the  character  of  musical  tones.     (See  Chap.  IX.) 

Since  the  pitch  numbers  of  any  octave  are  always  twice  as  great  as  those  for 
f  the  next  deeper,  we  find  the  pitch  numbers  of  the  higher  tones  by  multiplying 
those  of  the  small  or  unaccented  octave  as  many  times  by  2  as  its  symbol  has 
upper  accents.  And  on  the  contrary  the  pitch  numbers  for  the  deeper  octaves  are 
found  by  dividing  those  of  the  great  octave,  as  often  as  its  symbol  has  lower 
accents. 

Thus  C"=2X2XC=2X2X2(7 

C  =  ixixC  =  ixix^c. 

For  the  pitch  of  the  musical  scale  German  physicists  have  generally  adopted 
that  proposed  by  Scheibler,  and  adopted  subsequently  by  the  German  Association 
of  Natural  Philosophers  {die  deutsche  Naturforscherversammlung)  in  1834.  This 
makes  the  once-accented  a'  execute  440  vibrations  in  a  second.*    Hence  results  the 


thus  B^dos  (L'Art  du  Facteurd'Orgues,  1766) 
I  made  it  4  old  French  feet,  which  gave  a 
'  note  a  full  Semitone  flatter  than  a  pipe  of 
4  English  feet.  But  in  modern  organs  not  even 
80  much  as  4  English  feet  are  used.  Organ 
builders,  however,  in  all  countries  retain  the 
names  of  the  octaves  as  here  given,  which 
must  be  considered  merely  to  determine  the 
place  on  the  staff,  as  noted  in  the  text,  inde- 
pendently of  the  precise  pitch. — Translator,] 

*  The  Paris  Academy  has  lately  fixed  the 
pitch  number  of  the  same  note  at  435.  This 
is  called  870  by  the  Academy,  because  French 
physicists  have  adopted  the  inconvenient 
habit  of  counting  the  forward  motion  of  a 
swinging  body  as  one  vibration,  and  the  back- 
ward as  another,  so  that  the  whole  vibra- 
tion is  counted  as  two.  This  method  of 
counting  has  been  taken  from  the  seconds 
pendulimi,  which  ticks  once  in  going  forward 
and  once  again  on  returning.  For  symmetrical 


backward  and  forward  motions  it  would  be 
indifferent  by  which  method  we  counted,  but 
for  non-symmetrical  musical  vibrations  which 
are  of  constant  occurrence,  the  French  method 
of  counting  is  very  inconvenient.  The  number 
440  gives  fewer  fractions  for  the  first  (just] 
major  scale  of  C,  than  a' «  435.  The  difference 
of  pitch  is  less  than  a  comma.  [The  practical 
settlement  of  pitch  has  no  relation  to  such 
arithmetical  considerations  as  are  here  sug- 
gested, but  depends  on  the  compass  of  the 
human  voice  and  the  music  written  for  it  at 
different  times.  An  Abstract  of  my  History 
of  Musical  Pitch  is  given  in  Appendix  XX. 
sect.  H.  Scheibler's  proposal,  named  in  the 
text,  was  chosen,  as  he  tells  us  (Der  Tonmesser^ 
'834,  p.  53),  as  being  the  mean  between  the 
limits  of  pitch  within  which  Viennese  piano- 
fortes at  that  time  rose  and  fell  by  heat  and 
cold,  which  he  reckons  at  '^  vibration  either 
way.  That  this  proposal  had  no  reference  to  the 


Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


CHAP.    I. 


PITCH  NUMBERS  IN  JUST  MAJOR  SCALE. 


17 


following  table  for  the  scale  of  C  major,  which  will  serve  to  determine  the  pitch 
of  all  tones  that  are  defined  by  their  pitch  numbers  in  the  following  work. 


Unaccented 
Octave 

Once- 

Twice- 

Thrice- 

Four-times 

OontraOotaTe 

Great  Ootare 

accented 

accented 

aooented 

accented 

Notes 

C,UiB, 

CtoB 

Octave 

Octave 

Octave 

Octave 

x6foot 

8  foot 

£  to  & 

c'toy 

c"toft" 

e"'  to  V" 

&"'  to  y" 

afoot 

I  foot 

ifoot 

ifoot 

c 

33 

66 

132 

264 

528 

1056 
1 188 

2II2 

D 

37-125 

74-25 

148-5 

297 

594 

2376 

E 

4125 

825 

165 

330 

660 

1320 

2640 

F 

44 

88 

176 

352 

704 

1408 

2816 

0 

49-5 

99 

198 

396 

IV 

1584 

3168 

A 

55 

no 

220 

440 

880 

1760 

3520 

B 

61-875 

12375 

247-5 

495 

990 

1980 

3960* 

The  lowest  tone  on  orchestral  instruments  is  the  E,  of  the  double  bass,  making 
41^  vibrations  in  a  second. f    Modem  pianofortes  and  organs  usually  go  down  to  C^  % 


expression  of  the  jast  major  scale  in  \7h0le 
nambers,  is  shewn  by  the  fact  that  he 
proposed  it  for  an  equally  tempered  scale, 
for  which  he  calculated  the  pitch  numbers 
to  four  places  of  decimals,  and  for  which,  of 
course,  none  but  the  octaves  of  a'  are  ex- 
pressible by  whole  numbers.— TmtuZa^.] 

*  [As  it  is  important  that  students  should 
be  able  to  hear  the  exact  intervals  and  pitches 
spoken  of  throughout  this  book,  and  as  it  is 
quite  impossible  to  do  so  on  any  ordinary  in- 
strument, I  have  contrived  a  specially-tuned 
harmonium,  called  an  Harmonical,  fully  de- 
scribed in  App.  XX.  sect.  F.  No.  i,  which 
Messrs.  Bfoore  &  Moore,  104  Bishopsgate  Street, 
will,  in  the  interests  of  science,  supply  to  order, 
for  the  moderate  sum  of  1655.  The  follow- 
ing are  the  pitch  numbers  of  the  first  four 
octaves,  the  tuning  of  the  fifth  octave  will  be 


explained  in  App.  XX.  sect.  F.  The  names  of 
the  notes  are  in  the  notation  of  the  latter  part 
of  Chap.  XIV.  below.  Bead  the  sign  D,  as 
*D  one,'  E^\>  as  *  one  E  flat,'  and  ^Bb  as 
*  seven  B  flat.'  In  playing  observe  that  D,  is 
on  the  ordinary  D)>  or  C%  digital,  and  that 
^B\>  is  on  the  ordinary  Gb  or  ^  digital,  and 
that  the  only  keys  in  which  chords  can  be 
played  are  C  major  and  C  minor,  with  the 
minor  chord  D^FA^  and  the  natural  chord  of 
the  Ninth  OE^  O  ^B  b2>.  The  mode  of  measuring 
intervals  by  ratios  and  cents  is  fully  explained 
hereafter,  and  the  results  are  added  for  con- 
venience of  reference.  The  pitches  of  cf  528, 
a'  440,  a''  b  422*4  and  ^l/b  462,  were  taken  from 
forks  very  carefully  tuned  by  myself  to  these 
numbers  of  vibrations,  by  means  of  my  unique 
series  of  forks  described  in  App.  XX.,  at  the  ^ 
end  of  sect.  B. 


Scale  of  the  Haruonicaii. 


Pitch  Nambers. 

fiatios 

Cents 

Notes 

8  foot 

4fbot 

afoot 

I  foot 

Note  to  Note 

C  to  Note 

Note  to  Note 

CtoNote 

C 

66 

132 

264 

528 

9:10 

I :  I 

182 

0 

A 

73i 

146! 

293i 

586I 

80:81 

9:10 

22 

182 

D 

74i 

H^ 

297 

594 

15:16 

8:9 

112 

204 

^b 

79i 

1581 

316J 

633! 

24:25 

5:6 

70 

316 

^. 

82} 

165 

330 

660 

15:16 

4:5 

112 

386 

F 

88 

176 

352 

704 

8T9 

3:4 

204 

498 

G 

99 

198 

396 

792 

15:16 

2:3 

112 

702 

A^b 

io5f 

211J 

422f 

844! 

24:25 

5:8 

70 

814 

^1 

no 

220 

440 

880 

20:21 

3:5 

85 

884 

'Bb 

ii5i 

231 

462 

924 

35-36 

4:7 

49 

969 

B«b 

ii8| 

237! 

475i 

950I 

24:25 

5:9 

70 

IOI8 

Bi 

123I 

247i 

495 

990 

15:16 

8:15 

112 

1088 

C 

132 

264 

528 

1056 

— 

1:2 

— 

1200 

t  [The  following  account  of  the  actual  tones 
^>Md  is  adapted  from  my  History  of  Musical 


Translator.] 

Pitch.    C„t  commencement  of  the  32-foot  oc- 
tave, the  lowest  tone  of  very  large  organs,  two 

Digitized  by  V^OOQ IC 


i8 


COMPASS  OP  INSTRUMENTS. 


PART    I, 


with  33  vibrations,  and  the  latest  grand  pianos  even  down  to  A,,  with  27J  vibra- 
tions. On  larger  organs,  as  already  mentioned,  there  is  also  a  deeper  Octave  reach- 
ing to  Cf,  with  1 6^  vibrations.  But  the  musical  character  of  all  these  tones  below  E, 
is  imperfect,  because  we  are  here  near  to  the  limit  of  the  power  of  the  ear  to  combine 
vibrations  into  musical  tones.  These  lower  tones  cannot  therefore  be  used  musically 
^  except  in  connection  with  their  higher  octaves  to  which  they  impart  a  character 
of  greater  depth  without  rendering  the  conception  of  the  pitch  indeterminate. 

Upwards,  pianofortes  generally  reach  a""  with  3520,  or  evenc'  with  4224  vibra- 
tions. The  highest  tone  in  the  orchestra  is  probably  the  five-times  accented  d*  of  the 
piccolo  flute  with  4752  vibrations.  Appunn  and  W.  Preyer  by  means  of  small 
tuning-forks  excited  by  a  violin  bow  have  even  reached  the  eight  times  accented  c^*" 
with  40,960  vibraticHis  in  a  second.  These  high  tones  were  very  painfully  unplea- 
sant, and  the  pitch  of  those  which  exceed  the  boundaries  of  the  musical  scale  was 
^  very  imperfectly  discriminated  by  musical  observers.*    More  on  this  in  Chap.  IX. 

The  musical  tones  which  can  be  used  with  advantage,  and  have  clearly  dis- 
tinguishable pitch,  have  therefore  between  40  and  4000  vibrations  in  a  second, 
extending  over  7  octaves.  Those  which  are  audible  at  all  have  from  20  to  40,000 
vibrations,  extending  over  about  1 1  octaves.  This  shews  what  a  great  variety  of 
different  pitch  numbers  can  be  perceived  and  distinguished  by  the  ear.  In  this 
y/^  I  respect  the  ear  is  far  superior  to  the  eye,  which  likewise  distinguishes  light  of  dif- 
1  ferent  periods  of  vibration  by  the  sensation  of  different  colours,  for  tlie  compass  of 
the  vibrations  of  light  distinguishable  by  the  eye  but  slightly  exceeds  an  Octave.-h 

Force  and  pitch  were  the  two  first  differences  which  we  found  between  musical 
tones ;  the  third  was  quality  of  tonCj  which  we  have  now  to  investigate.     When 

of  Tone,*  (liber  die  Qrenzen  der  Tontcahmeh- 
mvngt  1876,  p.  20),  are  in  the  South  Kensing- 
ton Museum,  Scientific  Collection.  I  have 
several  times  tried  them.  I  did  not  myself 
find  the  tones  painful  or  cutting,  probably 
because  there  was  no  beating  of  inharmonic 
upper  partials.  It  is  best  to  sound  them  with 
two  violin  bows,  one  giving  the  octave  of  the 
other.  The  tones  can  be  easily  heard  at  a 
distance  of  more  than  100  feet  in  the  gallery 
of  the  Museum. — Translator,'] 

t  [Assuming  the  undulatory  theory,  which 
attributes  the  sensation  of  light  to  the  vibra- 
tions of  a  supposed  luminous  *  ether,'  resem- 
bling air  but  more  delicate  and  mobile,  then 
the  phenomena  of  *  interference '  enables  us 
to  calculate  the  lengths  of  waves  of  light  in 
empty  space,  <&c.,  hence  the  numbers  of  vibra- 
tions in  a  second,  and  consequently  the  ratios 
of  these  numbers,  which  will  then  clearly 
resemble  the  ratios  of  the  pitch  numbers  that 
measure  musical  intervals.  Assuming,  then, 
that  the  yellow  of  the  spectrum  answers  to  the 
tenor  c  in  music,  and  Fraunhofer*8  *  line  A  ' 
corresponds  to  the  Q  below  it.  Prof.  Helm- 
holtz,  in  his  Physiological  Optics^  (Ha^id- 
buck  der  physiologtschen  Optik,  1867,  p.  237), 
gives  the  f oUowing  analogies  between  the  notes 
of  the  piano  and  the  colours  of  the  spectrum  : — 
Fj  ,  end  of  the  Red.  /«  ,  Violet. 
G,Red. 


Octaves  below  the  lowest  tone  of  the  Violon- 
cello. A,^i  the  lowest  tone  of  the  largest 
pianos.  C.,  commencement  of  the  16-foot 
octave,  the  lowest  note  assigned  to  the  Double 
Bass  in  Beethoven's  Pastorid  Symphony.  £^, 
the  lowest  tone  of  the  German  four-stringed 
Double  Bass,  the  lowest  tone  mentioned  in 
the  text.  F„  the  lowest  tone  of  the  English 
four-stringed  Double  Bass.  0„  the  lowest  tone 
of  the  Italian  three-stringed  Double  Bass.  ^,, 
the  lowest  tone  of  the  English  three-stringed 
Double  Bass.  C,  commencement  of  the  8-foot 
octave,  the  lowest  tone  of  the  Violoncello, 
written  on  the  second  leger  line  below  the  bass 
stafiF.  G,  the  tone  of  the  third  open  string  of 
the  Violoncello,  c,  commencement  of  the 
4 -foot  octave  '  tenor  C,*  the  lowest  tone  of  the 
Vi61a,  written  on  the  second  space  of  the  bass 
staff,  d^  the  tone  of  the  second  open  string  of 
the  Violoncello.  /,  the  tone  signified  by  the 
bass  or  J^-clef.  ^,  the  lowest  tone  of  the 
Violin,  a,  the  tone  of  the  highest  open  string 
[  of  the  Violoncello.  c\  commencement  of  the 
2-foot  octave,  *  middle  C,'  written  on  the  leger 
line  between  the  bass  and  treble  staves,  the  tone 
signified  by  the  tenor  or  C-clef .  d',  the  tone  of  the 
third  open  string  of  the  Violin.  (/',  the  tone 
signified  by  the  treble  or  G-clef .  a',  the  tone  of 
the  second  open  string  of  the  Violin,  the  *  tuning 
note  *  for  orchestras,  c'^  commencement  of  the 
I  -foot  octave,  the  usual  *  tuning  note '  for  pianos. 
e't  the  tone  of  the  first  or  highest  open  string  of 
the  Violin,  c"',  commencement  of  the  ^-foot 
octave,  g*^,  the  usual  highest  tone  of  the 
Flute,  c'*,  commencement  of  the  |-foot  octave, 
e'*,  the  highest  tone  on  the  Violin,  being  the 
double  Octave  harmonic  of  the  tone  of  the 
highest  open  string,  a''',  the  usual  highest 
tone  of  large  pianos,  d",  the  highest  tone  of 
the  piccolo  flute,  e^",  the  highest  tone  reached 
by  Appunn 's  forks,  see  next  note. — Translator.} 
♦  [Copies  of  these  forks,  described  in  Prof. 
Preyer's  essay  •  On  the  Limits  of  the  Perception 


Gt ,  Red. 

A,  Bed. 

AZ ,  Orange-red. 

Bt  Orange. 

c,  Yellow. 

c9 ,  Green. 

dy  Greenish-blue. 

dt ,  Cyanogen-blue. 

e,  Indigo-blue. 

/,  Violet. 


g,  Ultra-violet. 

g%^       fi 

a,  „ 

at  J 

6,  end  of  the  solar 
spectrum. 

The  scale  there- 
fore extends  to 
about  a  Fourth 
beyond  the  oc- 
tave. —  Transla- 
tor.^ 


Digitized  by  VjOOQlC 


CHAP.  I.      QUALITY   OF  TONE   AND  FORM   OF   VIBRATION.  19 

we  hear  notes  of  the  same  force  and  same  pitch  sounded  successively  on  a  piano- 
forte, a  violin,  clarinet,  oboe,  or  trumpet,  or  by  the  human  voice,  the  character  of 
the  musical  tone  of  each  of  these  instruments,  notwithstanding  the  identity  of  force 
and  pitch,  is  so  different  that  by  means  of  it  we  recognise  with  the  greatest  ease 
which  of  these  instruments  w^as  used.  Varieties  of  quality  of  tone  appear  to  he 
infinitely  numerous.  Not  only  do  we  know  a  long  series  of  musical  instruments 
which  could  each  produce  a  note  of  the  same  pitch ;  not  only  do  different  individual 
instruments  of  the  same  species,  and  the  voices  of  different  individual  singers  shew 
certain  more  delicate  shades  of  quality  of  tone,  which  our  ear  is  able  to  distinguish  ; 
but  notes  of  the  same  pitch  can  sometimes  be  sounded  on  the  same  instrument  with 
several  qualitative  varieties.  In  this  respect  the  *  bowed  '  instruments  (i.e.  those 
of  the  violin  kind)  are  distinguished  above  all  other.  But  the  human  voice  is  still 
richer,  and  human  speech  employs  these  very  qualitative  varieties  of  tone,  in  order 
to  distinguish  different  letters.  The  different  vowels,  namely,  belong  to  the  class  ^ 
of  sustained  tones  which  can  be  used  in  music,  while  the  character  of  consonants 
mainly  depends  upon  brief  and  transient  noises. 

On  inquiring  to  what  external  physical  difference  in  the  waves  of  sound  the 
different  qualities  of  tone  correspond,  we  must  remember  that  the  amplitude  of 
the  vibration  determines  the  force  or  loudness,  and  the  period  of  vibration  the 
pitch.  Quality  of  tone  can  therefore  depend  upon  neither  of  these.  The  only 
possible  hypothesis,  therefore,  is  that  the  quality  of  tone  should  depend  upon  the 
manner  in  which  the  motion  is  performed  within  the  period  of  each  single  vibra- 
tion. For  the  generation  of  a  musical  tone  we  have  only  required  that  the  motion 
should  be  periodic,  that  is,  that  in  any  one  single  period  of  vibration  exactly  the 
same  state  should  occur,  in  the  same  order  of  occurrence  as  it  presents  itself  in  any 
other  single  period.  As  to  the  kind  of  motion  that  should  take  place  witliin  any 
single  period,  no  hypothesis  was  made.  In  this  respect  then  an  endless  variety  of 
motions  might  be  possible  for  the  production  of  sound.  % 

Observe  instances,  taking  first  such  periodic  motions  as  are  performed  so  slowly 
that  we  can  follow  them  with  the  eye.  Take  a  pendulum,  which  we  can  at  any 
time  construct  by  attaching  a  weight  to  a  thread  and  setting  it  in  motion.  The 
pendulum  swings  from  right  to  left  with  a  uniform  motion,  uninterrupted  by  jerks. 
Near  to  either  end  of  its  path  it  moves  slowly,  and  in  the  middle  fast.  Among 
sonorous  bodies,  which  move  in  the  same  way,  only  very  much  faster,  we  may 
mention  tuning-forks.  When  a  tuning-fork  is  struck  or  is  excited  by  a  violin  bow, 
and  its  motion  is  allowed  to  die  away  slowly,  its  two  prongs  oscillate  backwards 
and  forwards  in  the  same  way  and  after  the  same  law  as  a  pendulum,  only  they 
make  many  hundred  swings  for  each  single  swing  of  the  pendulum. 

As  another  example  of  a  periodic  piotion,  take  a  hammer  moved  by  a  water- 
wheel.  It  is  slowly  raised  by  the  millwork,  then  released,  and  falls  down  suddenly, 
is  then  again  slowly  raised,  and  so  on.  Here  again  we  have  a  periodical  backwards 
and  forwards  motion  ;  but  it  is  manifest  that  this  kind  of  motion  i»  totally  different  ^ 
from  that  of  the  pendulum.  Among  motions  which  produce  musical  sounds,  that  of 
a  violin  string,  excited  by  a  bow,  would  most  nearly  correspond  with  the  hammer's, 
as  will  be  seen  from  the  detailed  description  in  Chap.  V.  The  string  clings  for  a 
time  to  the  bow,  and  is  carried  along  by  it,  then  suddenly  releases  itself,  like  the 
hammer  in  the  mill,  and,  like  the  latter,  retreats  somewhat  with  much  greater 
Telocity  than  it  advanced,  and  is  again  caught  by  the  bow  and  carried  forward. 

Again,  imagine  a  ball  thrown  up  vertically,  and  caught  on  its  descent  with  a 
blow  which  sends  it  up  again  to  the  same  height,  and  suppose  this  operation  to  be 
performed  at  equal  intervals  of  time.  Such  a  ball  would  occupy  the  same  time  in 
rising  as  in  falling,  but  at  the  lowest  point  its  motion  would  be  suddenly  interrupted, 
whereas  at  the  top  it  would  pass  through  gradually  diminishing  speed  of  ascent 
into  a  gradually  increasing  speed  of  descent.  This  then  would  be  a  third  kind  of 
alternating  periodic  motion,  and  would  take  place  in  a  manner  essentially  different 
from  the  other  two*  ^ 

Digitized  by  VjOOQ  IC 


20 


FORM  OF  VIBRATION. 


PAUT  t. 


To  render  the  law  of  such  motions  more  comprehensible  to  the  eye  than  is 
possible  by  lengthy  verbal  descriptions,  mathematicians  and  physicists  are  in  the 
habit  of  applying  a  graphical  method,  which  must  be  frequently  employed  in  this 
work,  and  should  therefore  be  well  understood. 

To  render  this  method  intelligible  suppose  a  drawing  point  b,  fig.  5,  to  be 
fastened  to  the  prong  A  of  a  tuning-fork  in  such  a  manner  as  to  mark  a  surface 
of  paper  B  B.  Let  the  tuning-fork  be  moved  with  a  uniform  velocity  in  the  direc- 
tion of  the  upper  arrow,  or  else  the  paper  be  drawn  under  it  in  the  opposite 
direction,  as  shewn  by  the  lower  arrow.  When  the  fork  is  not  sounding,  the  point 
will  describe  the  dotted  straight  line  d  c.  But  if  the  prongs  have  been  first  set  in 
vibration,  the  point  will  describe  the  undulating  hne  d  c,  for  as  the  prong  vibrates, 
the  attached  point  b  will  constantly  move  backwards  and  forwards,  and  hence  be 


sometimes  on  the  right  and  sometimes  on  the  left  of  the  dotted  straight  line  d  c,  ad 
is  shewn  by  the  wavy  line  in  the  figure.  This  wavy  line  once  drawn,  remains  as  a 
permanent  image  of  the  kind  of  motion  performed  by  the  end  of  the  fork  during 
its  musical  vibrations.  As  the  point  b  is  moved  in  the  direction  of  the  straight 
line  d  c  with  a  constant  velocity,  equal  sections  of  the  straight  line  d  c  will  corre> 
spond  to  equal  sections  of  the  time  during  which  the  motion  lasts,  and  the  distance 
of  the  wavy  line  on  either  side  of  the  straight  line  will  shew  how  far  the  point  b 
lias  moved  from  its  mean  position  to  one  side  or  the  other  during  those  sections  of 
time. 

In  actually  performing  such  an  experiment  as  this,  it  is  best  to  wrap  the  paper 
over  a  cylinder  which  is  made  to  rotate  uniformly  by  clockwork.  The  paper  is 
wetted,  and  then  passed  over  a  turpentine  flame  which  coats  it  with  lampblack, 
on  which  a  fine  and  somewhat  smooth  steel  point  will  easily  trace  delicate  lines. 

Via.  6. 


Fig.  6  is  tlie  copy  of  a  drawing  actually  made  in  this  way  on  the  rotating  cylinder 
of  Messrs.  Scott  and  Koenig's  PJionautograph. 

Fig.  7  shews  a  portion  of  this  curve  on  a  larger  scale.  It  is  easy  to  see  the 
meaning  of  such  a  curve.  The  drawing  point  has  passed  with  a  uniform  velocity 
in  the  direction  e  h.  Suppose  that  it  has  described  the  section  e  g  in  jV  ^^  ^ 
second.  Divide  e  g  into  12  equal  parts,  as  in  the  figure,  then  the  point  has  been 
j^jf  of  a  second  in  describing  the  length  of  any  such  section  horizontally,  and 
the  curve  shews  us  on  what  side  and  at  what  distance  from  the  position  of 
rest  the  vibrating  point  will  be  at  the  end  of  -j-^,  y^,  and  so  on,  of  a  second, 
or,  generally,  at  any  given  short  interval  of  time  since  it  left  the  point  e. 
We  see,  in  the  figure,  that  after  j^ir  ^^  &  second  it  had  reached  the  height  i, 
and  that  it  rose  gradually  till  tlie  end  of  y^^  of  a  second ;  then,  however,  it  began 
to  descend  gradually  till,  at  the  end  of  j^  =  ^V  second,  it  had  reached  its  mean 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQIC 


CHAP.  I. 


FORM  OP  VIBRATION. 


91 


position  f,  and  then  it  oontinued  descending  on  the  opposite  side  till  the  end  of 
tI^f  of  a  second  and  so  on.  We  can  also  easily  detennine  where  the  vibrating 
point  was  to  be  found  at  the  end  of  any  fraction  of  this  hundred-and-twentieth  of 
a  second.  A  drawing  of  this  kind  consequently  shews  immediately  at  what  point  of 
its  path  a  vibrating  particle  is  to  be  found  at  any  given  instant,  and  hence  gives  a 
complete  image  of  its  motion.  If  the  reader  wishes  to  reproduce  the  motion  of  the 
vibrating  point,  he  has  only  to  cut  a  narrow  vertical  sUt  in  a  piece  of  paper,  and 
place  it  over  fig,  6  or  fig.  7,  so  as  to  shew  a  very  small  portion  of  the  curve  through 
the  vertical  slit,  and  draw  the  book  slowly  but  uniformly  under  the  slit,  from  right 
to  left ;  the  white  or  black  point  in  the  sht  will  then  appear  to  move  backwards  and 
forwards  in  precisely  the  same  manner  as  the  original  drawing  point  attached  to 
the  fork,  only  of  course  much  more  slowly. 

We  are  not  yet  able  to  make  all  vibrating  bodies  describe  their  vibrations 


Fio, 


H 


directly  on  paper,  although  much  progress  has  recently  been  made  in  the 
methods  required  for  this  purpose.  But  we  are  able  ourselves  to  draw  such 
curves  for  all  sounding  bodies,  when  the  law  of  their  motion  is  known,  that  is, 
when  we  know  how  far  the  vibrating  point  will  be  from  its  mean  position  at  any 
given  moment  of  time.  We  then  set  off  on  a  horiEontal  line,  such  as  e  f,  fig.  7, 
lengths  corresponding  to  the  interval  of  time,  and  let  fall  perpendiculars  to  it  on  ^ 
either  side,  making  their  lengths  equal  or  proportional  to  the  distance  of  the  vibrat- 
ing point  from  its  mean  position,  and  then  by  joining  the  extremities  of  these  per- 
pendiculars we  obtain  a  curve  such  as  the  vibrating  body  would  have  drawn  if  it 
had  been  possible  to  make  it  do  so. 

Thus  fig.  8  represents  the  motion  of  the  hammer  raised  by  a  water-wheel,  or  of 
a  point  in  a  string  excited  by  a  vioUn  bow.  For  the  first  9  intervals  it  rises  slowly 
and  uniformly,  and  during  the  loth  it  falls  suddenly  down. 


Fig.  8. 


Fig. 


10 


Fig.  9  represents  the  motion  of  the  ball  which  is  struck  up  again  as  soon  as  it  f 
comes  down.    Ascent  and  descent  are  performed  with  equal  rapidity,  whereas  in 
fig.  8  the  ascent  takes  much  longer  time.  But  at  the  lowest  point  the  blow  suddenly 
changes  the  kind  of  motion. 

Physicists,  then,  having  in  their  mind  such  curvilinear  forms,  representing  the 
law  of  the  motion  of  sounding  bodies,  speak  briefly  of  the  form  of  vihration  of  a 
sounding  body,  and  assert  that  the  quality  of  tone  depends  on  the  form  of  vibration. 
This  assertion,  which  has  hitherto  been  based  simply  on  the  fact  of  our  knowing 
that  the  quality  of  the  tone  could  not  possibly  depend  on  the  periodic  time  of  a 
vibration,  or  on  its  ampUtude  (p.  loc),  will  be  strictly  examined  hereafter.  It 
will  be  shewn  to  be  in  so  far  correct  that  every  different  quality  of  tone  requires  a 
different  form  of  vibration,  but  on  the  other  hand  it  wiU  also  appear  that  different 
forms  of  vibration  may  correspond  to  the  same  quality  of  tone. 

On  exactly  and  carefully  examining  the  effect  produced  on  the  ear  by  different 
forms  of  vibration,  as  for  example  that  in  fig.  8,  corresponding  nearly  to  a  violin 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


22-  COMPOUND  AND  PARTIAL  TONES.  pabt  i. 

string,  we  meet  with  a  strange  and  unexpected  phenomenon,  long  known  indeed  to 
indi\ddual  musicians  and  physicists,  but  commonly  regarded  as  a  mere  curiosity, 
its  generality  and  its  great  significance  for  all  matters  relating  to  musical  tones  not 
having  been  recognised.  The  ear  when  its  attention  has  been  properly  directed  to 
the  effect  of  the  vibrations  which  strike  it,  does  not  hear  merely  that  one  musical 
tone  whose  pitch  is  determined  by  the  period  of  the  vibrations  in  the  manner 
already  explained,  but  in  addition  to  this  it  becomes  aware  of  a  whole  series  of 
higher  musical  tones,  which  we  will  call  the  harmonic  upper  partial  tones^  and 
sometimes  simply  the  upper  partials  of  the  whole  musical  tone  or  note,  in  contra- 
distinction  to  the  fundarmntal  or  prime  partial  tone  or  simply  the  prime^  as  it  may 
be  called,  which  is  the  lowest  and  generally  the  loudest  of  all  the  partial  tones,  and 
by  the  pitch  of  which  we  judge  of  the  pitch  of  the  whole  compound  musical  tone 
itself.  The  series  of  these  upper  partial  tones  is  precisely  the  same  for  all  com- 
H  pound  musical  tones  which  correspond  to  a  uniformly  periodical  motion  of  the  air. 
It  is  as  follows  : — 

The  first  upper  partial  tone  [or  second  partial  tone]  is  the  upper  Octave  of  the 
prime  tone,  and  makes  double  the  number  of  vibrations  in  the  same  time.  If  we 
call  the  prime  0,  this  upper  Octave  will  be  c. 

The  second  upper  partial  tone  [or  third  partial  tone]  is  the  Fifth  of  this  Octave, 
or  g,  making  three  times  as  many  vibrations  in  the  same  time  as  the  prime. 

The  third  upper  partial  tone  [or  fourth  partial  tone]  is  the  second  higher  Octave, 
or  c\  making  four  times  as  many  vibrations  as  the  prime  in  the  same  time. 

The  fourth  upper  partial  tone  [or  fifth  partial  tone]  is  the  major  Third  of  this 
second  higher  Octave,  or  e\  with  five  times  as  many  vibrations  as  the  prime  in  tlie 
same  time. 

The  fifth  upper  partial  tone  [or  sixth  partial  tone]  is  the  Fifth  of  the  second 
higher  Octave,  or  g\  making  six  times  as  many  vibrations  as  the  prime  in  the 
^  same  time. 

And  thus  they  go  on,  becoming  continually  fainter,  to  tones  making  7,  8,  9, 
&c.,  times  as  many  vibrations  in  the  same  time,  as  the  prime  tone.  Or  in  musical 
notation 


^^ 


-^ 


'^  c     g  c'    e'    g'    »6't)  c"  d"  «"  "/'  g"  'V'  '6"t>  h" 


9 

c'    e'   g'    »6't?  c"  d"  e"  "/'  g"  »V 

3 

456        7       8     9    10    II      12    13 

198 

464330396    462    528594660726  792858 

c" 


omfnanjnmberof,      ^        3  4       5      6  7         »      9     lO     II       12     13       I4       15       16 

Pitch  number    66  132    198  264330396     462    528594660726   792858     924    990   105^^* 

where  the  figures  [in  the  first  line]  beneath  shew  how  many  times  the  corresponding 
pitch  number  is  greater  than  that  of  the  prime  tone  [and,  taking  the  lowest  note 
to  have  66  vibrations,  those  in  the  second  hne  give  the  pitch  numbers  of  all  the 
H  other  notes]. 

The  whole  sensation  excited  in  the  ear  by  a  periodic  vibration  of  the  air  we 

*  [This  diagram  has  been  slightly  altered  to  This  slightly  flattens  each  note,  and  slow  beats 
introduce  all  the  first  i6  harmonic  partials  can  be  prodaced  in  every  case  (except,  of 
of  C  66,  (which,  excepting  ii  and  13,  are  course,  11  and  13,  which  are  not  on  the 
given  on  the  Harmonical  as  harmonic  notes,)  instrument)  up  to  16.  It  should  also  be  ob- 
and  to  shew  the  notation,  symbolising,  both  in  served  that  the  pitch  of  the  beat  is  very  nearly 
letters  and  on  the  staff,  the  7th,  nth,  and  that  of  the  upper  (not  the  lower)  note  in  each 
13th  harmonic  partials,  which  are  not  used  in  case.  The  whole  of  these  16  harmonics  of  C66 
general  music.  It  is  easy  to  shew  on  the  (except  the  nth  and  13th)  can  be  played 
Harmonical  that  its  lowest  note,  G  of  this  at  once  on  the  Harmonical  by  means  of  the 
scries,  contains  all  these  partials,  after  the  harmonical  bar,  first  without  and  then  with 
theory  of  the  beats  of  a  disturbed  unison  the  7th  and  14th.  The  whole  series  will  be 
has  been  explained  in  Chap.  VIII.  Keep  found  to  sound  like  a  single  fine  note,  and  the 
down  the  note  C,  and  touch  in  sucoessioi;!  the  7th  and  14th  to  materially  increase  its  rich- 
notes  c,  <7,  c\  e',  g\  A-c,  but  in  touching  the  latter  ness.  The  relations  of  the  partials  in  this  case 
press  the  finger-key  such  a  little  way  down  may  be  studied  from  the  tables  in  the  footnotes 
that  the  tone  of  the  note  is  only  just  audible.  to  Chap.  X.  -  Translator.] 


Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


CHAP.   I. 


DEFINITION  OF  TERMS  EMPLOYED. 


23 


have  called  a  musical  tone.  We  now  find  tbat  this  is  compound^  containing  a 
series  of  different  tones,  which  we  distinguish  as  the  constitutents  or  partial  tones 
of  the  compound*  The  first  of  these  constituents  is  the  prvme  partial  tone  of  the 
compound,  and  the  rest  its  harmonic  upper  partial  tones.  The  number  which 
shews  the  order  of  any  partial  tone  in  the  series  shews  how  many  times  its 
vibrational  number  exceeds  that  of  the  prime  tone.*  Thus,  the  second  partial 
tone  makes  twice  as  many,  the  third  three  times  as  many  vibrations  in  the  same 
lime  as  the  prime  tone,  and  so  on. 

6.  S.  Ohm  was  the  first  to  declare  that  there  is  only  one  form  of  vibration 
which  will  give  rise  to  no  harmonic  upper  partial  tones,  and  which  will  therefore 
consist  solely  of  the  prime  tone.  This  is  the  form  of  vibration  which  we  have 
described  above  as  peculiar  to  the  pendulum  and  tuning-forks,  and  drawn  in  figs.  6 
and  7  (p.  10).  We  will  call  tliese  pendular  vibratio^is,  or,  since  they  cannot  be 
analysed  into  a  compound  of  different  tones,  siynple  vibrations.  In  what  sense  not  ^ 
merely  other  musical  tones,  but  all  other  forms  of  vibration,  may  be  considered 
as  compound,  will  be  shewn  hereafter  (Chap.  IV.).  The  terms  simple  ox  pendular 
vibration;^  wiU  therefore  be  used  as  synonymous.  We  have  hitherto  used  the 
expression  tone  and  musical  tone  indifferently.  It  is  absolutely  necessary  to  dis- 
tinguish in  acoustics  $rst,  a  musical  tofie,  that  is,  the  impression  made  by  ani/ 
periodical  vibration  of  the  air ;  secondly,  a  simple  tone,  that  is,  the  impression 
produced  by  a  simple  or  pendular  vibration  of  the  air ;  and  thirdly,  a  cofnpou?id 
tone,  that  is,  the  impression  produced  by  the  simultaneous  action  of  several  simple 
tones  with  certain  definite  ratios  of  pitch  as  already  explained.  A  musical  tone 
may  be  either  simple  or  compound.    For  the  sake  of  brevity,  tone  will  be  used  in 


*  [The  ordinal  number  of  a  partial  tone 
in  general,  must  be  distinguished  from  the 
ordinal  number  of  an  iqrper  partial  tone  in 
particular.  For  the  same  tone  the  former 
number  is  alvr-ays  greater  by  unity  than  the 
latter,  because  the  partials  in  general  include 
the  prime,  which  is  reckoned  as  the  first,  and 
the  upper  partials  exclude  the  prime,  which 
being  the  loicest  partial  is  of  course  not  an 
upper  partial  at  all.  Thus  the  partials  gene- 
rally numbered  23456789  are  the 
same  as  the  upper  partials  numbered  i  2  3 
45678  respectively.  As  even  the 
Author  has  occasionally  failed  to  carry  out 
this  distinction  in  the  original  German  text, 
and  other  writers  have  constantly  neglected  it, 
too  much  weight  cannot  be  here  laid  upon  it. 
The  presence  or  absence  of  the  word  upjyer 
before  the  word  partial  must  always  be  care- 


fully observed.  It  is  safer  never  to  speak  of 
an  upper  partial  by  its  ordinal  number,  but  to 
call  the  fifth  upper  partial  the  sixth  partial, 
omitting  the  word  upper  and  increasing  the  ^ 
ordinal  number  by  one  place.  And  so  in 
other  cases. — Translator,] 

t  The  law  of  these  vibrations  may  be 
popularly  explained  by  means  of  the  construc- 
tion in  fig.  10.  Suppose  a  point  to  describe 
the  circle  of  which  c  is  the  centre  with  a 
uniform  velocity,  and  that  an  observer  stands 
at  a  considerable  distance  in  the  prolongation 
of  the  line  e  h,  so  that  he  does  not  see  the 
surface  of  the  circle  but  only  its  edge,  in 
which  case  the  point  will  appear  merely  to 
move  up  and  down  along  its  diameter  a  b. 
This  up  and  down  motion  would  take  place 
exactly  according  to  the  law  of  pendular 
vibration.    To  represent  this  motion  graphi- 


FlG. 


cally  by  means  of  a  curve,  divide  the  length 
e  g,  supposed  to  correspond  to  the  time  of  a 
single  period,  into  as  many  (here  12)  equal 
parts  as  the  circumference  of  the  circle,  and 
draw  the  perpendiculars  i,  2,  3,  &c.,  on  the 
dividing  points  of  the  line  e  g,  in  order,  equal 
in  length  to  and  in  the  same  direction  with, 
those  drawn  in  the  circle  from  the  correspond- 
ing points  I,  2,  3,  <&c.  In  this  way  we  obtain 
ihe  curve  drawn  in  fig.  10,  which  agrees  in 


form  with  that  drawn  by  the  tuning-fork, 
tg.  6,  p.  206,  but  is  of  a  larger  size.  Mathe- 
matioidly  expressed,  the  distance  of  the  vibrat- 
ing point  from  its  mean  position  at  any  time 
is  equal  to  the  sine  of  an  arc  proportional  to 
the  corresponding  time,  and  henoe  the  form  of 
simple  vibrations  are  also  called  the  sin&- 
vibrati/yiis  [and  the  above  cui*ve  is  also  known 
as  the  curve  of  sinesl. 


Digitized  by  VjOOQIC 


24 


DEFINITION  OP  TERMS  EMPLOYED. 


PART   I. 


the  general  senae  of  a  musical  tone,  leaving  the  context  or  a  prefixed  qualification 
to  determine  whether  it  is  simple  or  compound.  A  compound  tone  will  often  be 
briefly  called  a  note,  and  a  simple  tone  will  also  be  frequently  cuXleA  tk  partial,  when 
used  in  connection  with  a  compound  tone ;  otherwise,  the  full  expression  simple 
tone  will  be  employed.  A  note  has,  properly  speaking,  no  single  pitch,  as  it  is 
made  up  of  various  partials  each  of  which  has  its  own  pitch.  By  the  pitch  of  a 
note  or  compound  tone  then  we  shall  therefore  mean  the  pitch  of  its  lowest  partial 
or  prime  tone.  By  a  chord  or  combination  of  tones  we  mean  several  musical  tones 
(whether  simple  or  compound)  produced  by  different  instruments  or  different  parts 
of  the  same  instrument  so  as  to  be  heard  at  the  same  time.  The  facts  here  adduced 
shew  us  then  that  every  musical  tone  in  which  harmonic  upper  partial  tones  can 
be  distinguished,  although  produced  by  a  single  instrument,  may  really  be  con- 
sidered as  in  itself  a  chord  or  combination  of  various  simple  tones.* 


If  ♦  [The  above  paragraph  relating  to  the 
Engliedi  terms  used  in  this  translation,  neces- 
sarily differs  in  many  respects  from  the  original, 
in  which  a  jnstification  is  given  of  the  use 
made  by  the  Author  of  certain  Oerman  ex- 
pressions. It  has  been  my  object  to  employ 
terms  which  should  be  thoroughly  English, 
and  should  not  in  any  way  recall  the  German 
words.  The  word  tone  in  English  is  extremely 
ambiguous.  Prof.  Tyndall  {Lectures  on  Sounds 
2nd  ed.  1869,  p.  117)  has  ventured  to  define  a 
tone  as  a  simple  tone^  in  agreement  with  Prof. 
Helmholtz,  who  in  the  present  passage  limits 
the  Grerman  word  Ton  in  the  same  way.  But 
I  felt  that  an  English  reader  could  not  be 
safely  trusted  to  keep  this  very  peculiar  and 
important  class  of  musical  tones,  which  he 
has  very  rarely  or  never  heard  separately, 
invariably  distinct  from  those  musical  tones 

%  with  which  he  is  familiar,  unless  the  word 
tone  were  uniformly  qualified  by  the  epithet 
simple.  The  only  exception  I  could  make  was 
in  tiie  case  of  a  partial  tone,  which  is  received 
at  once  as  a  new  conception.  Even  Prof. 
Helmholtz  himself  has  not  succeeded  in  using 
his  word  Ton  consistently  for  a  simple  tone 
only,  and  this  was  an  additional  warning  to 
me.  English  musicians  have  been  also  in 
the  habit  of  using  tone  to  signify  a  certain 
musical  interval,  and  semitone  for  half  of  that 
interval,  on  the  equally  tempered  scale.  In 
this  case  I  write  Tone  and  Semitone  with 
capital  initials,  a  practice  which,  as  already 
explained  (note,  p.  ijd',)  I  have  found  con- 
venient for  the  names  of  all  intervals,  as 
Thirds,  Fifths,  Ao.  Prof.  Hehnholtz  uses  the 
word  Klang  for  a  musical  tone,  which  gene- 

f  rally,  but  not  always,  means  a  compound  tone. 
Prof.  Tyndall  (ibid,)  therefore  proposes  to  use 
the  English  word  clang  in  the  same  sense. 
But  dang  has  already  a  meaning  in  English, 
thus  de&ied  by  Webster:  *a  sharp  shrill 
sound,  made  by  striking  together  metallic 
substances,  or  sonorous  bodies,  as  the  clang 
of  arms,  or  any  like  sound,  as  the  clang  of 
trumpets.  This  word  implies  a  degree  of 
harshness  in  the  sound,  or  more  harshness 
than  dink,*  Interpreted  scientifically,  then, 
clang  according  to  this  definition,  is  either 
noise  or  one  of  those  musical  tones  with  in- 
harmonic  upper  partials,  which  will  be  sub- 
sequently explained.  It  is  therefore  totally 
unadapted  to  represent  a  tus^^iual  tone  in 
generiU,  for  which  the  simple  word  tone  seems 
eminently  suited,'  being  of  course  originally 
the  tone  produced  by  a  stretched  string.  The 
common  word   note,  properly    the  mark   by 


which  a  musical  tone  is  written,  will  also,  in 
accordance  with  the  general  practice  of  musi- 
cians, be  used  for  a  musical  tone,  which  is 
generally  compound,  without  necessarily  im- 
plying that  it  is  one  of  the  few  reco^iised 
tones  in  our  musical  scale.  Of  oouree,  if 
dang  could  not  be  used.  Prof.  Tyndall's 
suggestion  to  translate  Prof.  Helmholtz's 
Klangfarbe  by  clangtiwt  (ibid,)  fell  to  the 
ground.  I  can  find  no  valid  season  for  sup- 
planting the  time-honoured  expression  gualtiy 
of  tone.  Prof.  Tyndall  (ibid,)  quotes  Dr. 
Young  to  the  effect  that  *  this  quality  of  sound 
is  sometimes  called  its  register,  colour,  ot 
timbre.'  Register  has  a  distinct  meaning  in 
vocal  music  which  must  not  be  disturbed. 
Timbre,  properly  a  kettledrum,  then  a  helmet, 
then  the  coat  of  arms  surmounted  with  a 
helmet,  then  the  official  stamp  bearing  that 
coat  of  arms  (now  used  in  France  for  a 
postage  label),  and  then  the  mark  which 
declared  a  thing  to  be  what  it  pretends  to  be, 
Bums*s  '  guinea's  stamp,'  is  a  foreign  word, 
often  odiously  mispronounced,  and  not  worth 
preserving.  Colour  I  have  never  met  with 
as  applied  to  music,  except  at  most  as  a 
passing  metaphorical  expression.  But  the 
difference  of  tones  in  quality  is  familiar  to 
our  language.  Then  as  to  the  Partial  Tones, 
Prof.  Helmholtz  uses  TheilUSne  and  Partial- 
tone,  which  are  aptly  Englished  by  partial 
simple  tones.  The  words  simple  and  tone, 
however,  may  be  omitted  when  partials  is 
employed,  as  partials  are  necessarily  both 
tones  and  simple.  The  constilv^nt  tones  of  a 
chord  may  be  either  simple  or  compound. 
The  Qrundton  or  fundamental  tone  of  a 
compound  tone  tiien  becomes  its  prims  tone, 
or  briefly  its  prime.  The  Grundton  or  root  of 
a  chord  will  be  further  explained  liereafter. 
Upper  partial  (simple)  tones,  that  is,  the 
partials  exclusive  of  the  prime,  even  when 
hoArmonic,  (that  is,  for  the  most  part,  belong- 
ing to  the  first  six  partial  tones,)  must  be 
distinguished  from  the  sounds  usually  called 
harmonics  when  produced  on  a  violin  or  harp 
for  instance,  for  such  harmonics  are  not  neces- 
sarily simple  tones,  but  are  more  generally 
compounds  of  soms  of  the  complete  series  of 
partial  tones  belonging  to  the  musical  tone  of 
the  whole  string,  selected  by  damping  the 
remainder.  The  fading  harmonics  heard  in 
listening  to  the  sound  of  a  pianoforte  string, 
struck  and  undamped,  as  the  sound  dies  away, 
are  also  compound  and  not  simple  partial 
tones,  but  as  they  have  the  successive  partials 
for  their  successive  primes,  they  have  the 
Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


CHAPS.  I.  II.    COEXISTENCE  OF  DISTINCT  WAVES  OF  SOUND.  25 

Now,  since  quality  of  tone,  as  we  have  seen,  depends  on  the  form  of  vibration, 
which  also  determines  the  occurrence  of  upper  partial  tones,  we  have  to  inquire 
how  &r  differences  in  quality  of  tone  depend  on  different  force  or  loudness  of  upper 
partials.  This  inquiry  will  be  found  to  give  a  means  of  clearing  up  our  concep- 
tions of  what  has  hitherto  been  a  perfect  enigma, — ^the  nature  of  quality  of  tone. 
And  we  must  then,  of  course,  attempt  to  explain  how  the  ear  manages  to  analyse 
every  musical  tone  into  a  series  of  partial  tones,  and  what  is  the  meaning  of  this 
analysis.    These  investigations  will  engage  our  attention  in  the  following  chapters. 


CHAPTER  II. 

OK   THE  COMPOSITION  OF  VIBBATIONS. 


At  the  end  of  the  last  chapter  we  came  upon  the  remarkable  fact  that  the  human 
ear  is  capable,  under  certain  conditions,  of  separating  the  musical  tone  produced 
by  a  single  musical  instrument,  into  a  series  of  simple  tones,  namely,  the  prime 
I«rtial  tone,  and  the  various  upper  partial  tones,  each  of  which  produces  its  own 
separate  sensation.  That  the  ear  is  capable  of  distinguishing  from  each  other 
tones  proceeding  from  different  sources,  that  is,  which  do  not  arise  from  one  and 
the  same  sonorous  body,  we  know  from  daily  experience.  There  is  no  difficulty 
during  a  concert  in  following  the  melodic  progression  of  each  individual  instru- 
ment or  voice,  if  we  direct  our  attention  to  it  exclusively ;  and,  after  some  practice, 
most  persons  can  succeed  in  following  the  simultaneous  progression  of  several 
united  parts.  This  is  true,  indeed,  not  merely  for  musical  tones,  but  also  for 
noises,  and  for  mixtures  of  music  and  noise.  When  several  persons  are  speaking 
at  once,  we  can  generally  listen  at  pleasure  to  the  words  of  any  single  one  of  them,  IT 
and  even  understand  those  words,  provided  that  they  are  not  too  much  overpowered 
by  the  mere  loudness  of  the  others.  Hence  it  foUows,  first,  that  many  different 
trains  of  waves  of  sound  can  be  propagated  at  the  same  time  through  the  same 
mass  of  air,  without  mutual  disturbance ;  and,  secondly,  that  the  human  ear  is 
capable  of  again  analysing  into  its  constituent  elements  that  composite  motion  of 
the  air  which  is  produced  by  the  simultaneous  action  of  several  musical  instru- 
ments. We  will  first  investigate  the  nature  of  the  motion  of  the  air  when  it  is 
produced  by  several  simultaneous  musical  tones,  and  how  such  a  compound  motion 
is  distinguished  from  that  due  to  a  single  musical  tone.  We  shall  see  that  the  ear 
has  no  decisive  test  by  which  it  can  in  all  cases  distinguish  between  the  effect  of  a 

pitch  of  those  partials.  But  these  fading  meaning  uppery  but  the  English  preposition 
harvKmics  are  not  regular  compound  tones  of  over  is  equivalent  to  the  German  preposition 
the  kind  described  on  p.  22a,  because  the  lower  fiber.  Compare  Obergahn,  an  '  upper  tooth/  f 
partials  are  absent  one  after  another.  Both  i.e.  a  tooth  in  the  upper  jaw»  with  Uebermhn^ 
sets  of  harmonics  serve  to  indicate  the  exist-  an  *  overtooth/  i.e.  one  grown  over  another, 
enoe  and  place  of  the  partials.  But  they  are  a  projecting  tooth.  The  continual  recurrence 
no  more  those  upper  partial  tones  themselves,  of  such  words  as  clang,  clangtifU,  overtone^ 
than  the  original  compound  tone  of  the  string  would  combine  to  give  a  strange  un-English 
is  its  own  prime.  Great  confusion  of  thought  appearance  to  a  translation  from  the  German, 
having,  to  my  own  knowledge,  arisen  from  Chi  the  contrary  I  have  endeavoured  to  put  it 
eonfounding  such  ^rmomc«  with  upper  partial  into  as  straightforward  English  as  possible. 
t€ne$,  I  have  generally  avoided  using  ^e  am-  But  for  those  acquainted  with  the  original  and 
biguons  substantive  ^rmonic.  Properly  speak-  with  Prof.  Tyndall's  work,  this  explanation 
ing  the  harmonics  of  any  compound  tone  are  seemed  necessary.  Finally  I  would  caution 
other  oompound  tones  of  which  the  primes  are  the  reader  against  using  overtones  for  partial 
partials  of  the  original  compound  tone  of  tones  in  general,  as  almost  every  one  who 
which  they  are  said  to  be  harmonics.  Prof.  adopts  Prof.  Tyndall's  word  is  in  the  habit  of 
Hehnholtz's  term  OberWne  is  merely  a  con-  doing.  Indeed  I  have  in  the  course  of  this 
traction  for  OberpartialtOne,  but  the  casual  translation  observed,  that  even  Prof.  Helmholtz 
resemblance  of  the  sounds  of  ober  and  over,  has  himself  has  been  occasionally  misled  to  em- 
led  Prof.  Tyndall  to  the  erroneous  translation  ploy  OberUitie  in  the  same  loose  manner.  See 
overtones.    The  German  obcr  is  an  adjective  my  remarks  in  note,  p.  23c.— TransZato-.l 

Digitized  by  VjOOvIc 


26.  COMPOSITION  OF  WAVES.  part  i. 

motion  of  the  air  caused  by  several  diflferent  musical  tones  arising  from  different 
sources,  and  that  caused  by  the  musical  tone  of  a  single  sounding  body.  Hence 
the  ear  has  to  analyse  the  composition  of  single  musical  tones,  under  proper  con- 
ditions, by  means  of  the  same  febculty  which  enabled  it  to  analyse  the  compositicHi 
of  simultaneous  musical  tones.  We  shall  thus  obtain  a  clear  conception  of  what 
is  meant  by  analysing  a  single  musical  tone  into  a  series  of  partial  simple  tones^ 
and  we  shall  perceive  that  this  phenomenon  depends  upon  one  of  the  most 
essential  and  fundamental  properties  of  the  human  ear. 

We  begin  by  examming  the  motion  of  the  air  which  corresponds  to  several 
simple  tones  acting  at  the  same  time  on  the  same  mass  of  air.  To  illustrate  this 
kind  of  motion  it  will  be  again  convenient  to  refer  to  the  waves  formed  on  a  calni 
surface  of  water.  We  have  seen  (p.  9a)  that  if  a  point  of  the  surface  is  agitated  by  a 
stone  thrown  upon  it,  the  agitation  is  propagated  in  rings  of  waves  over  the  surface 

If  1,0  more  and  more  distant  points.  Now,  throw  two  stones  at  the  same  time  on  to 
different  points  of  the  surface,  thus  producing  two  centres  of  agitation.  Each  will 
give  rise  to  a  separate  ring  of  waves,  and  the  two  rings  gradually  expanding,  will 
finally  meet.  Where  the  waves  thus  come  together,  the  water  will  be  set  in 
motion  by  both  kinds  of  agitation  at  the  same  time,  but  this  in  no  wise  prevents 
both  series  of  waves  from  advancing  further  over  the  surface,  just  as  if  each  were 
alone  present  and  the  other  had  no  existence  at  all.  As  they  proceed,  those 
parts  of  both  rings  which  had  just  coincided,  again  appear  separate  and  unaltered 
in  form.  These  httle  waves,  caused  by  throwing  in  stones,  may  be  accompanied 
by  other  kinds  of  waves,  such  as  those  due  to  the  wind  or  a  passing  steamboat. 
Our  circles  of  waves  will  spread  out  over  the  water  thus  agitated,  with  the  same 
quiet  regularity  as  they  did  upon  the  calm  surface.  Neitlier  wiU  the  greater  waves 
be  essentially  disturbed  by  the  less,  nor  the  less  by  the  greater,  provided  the  waves 
never  break ;  if  that  happened,  their  regular  course  would  certainly  be  impeded. 

%  Indeed  it  is  seldom  possible  to  survey  a  large  surface  of  water  from  a  high 
point  of  sight,  without  perceiving  a  great  multitude  of  different  systems  of  waves, 
mutually  overtopping  and  crossing  each  other.  This  is  best  seen  on  the  surface  of 
the  sea,  viewed  from  a  lofty  cHff,  when  there  is  a  lull  after  a  stiff  breeze.  We  first 
see  the  great  waves,  advancing  in  far-stretching  ranks  from  the  blue  distance,  here 
and  there  more  clearly  marked  out  by  their  white  foaming  crests,  and  following 
one  another  at  regular  intervals  towards  the  shore.  From  the  shore  they  rebound, 
in  different  directions  according  to  its  sinuosities,  and  cut  obliquely  across  the 
advancing  waves.  A  passing  steamboat  forms  its  own  wedge-shaped  wake  of 
waves,  or  a  bird,  darting  on  a  fish,  excites  a  small  circular  system.  The  eye  of  the 
spectator  is  easily  able  to  pursue  each  one  of  these  different  trains  of  waves,  great 
and  small,  wide  and  narrow,  straight  and  curved,  and  observe  how  each  passes 
over  the  surface,  as  undisturbedly  as  if  the  water  over  which  it  flits  were  not 
agitated  at  the  same  time  by  other  motions  and  other  forces.     I  must  own  that 

IT  whenever  I  attentively  observe  this  spectacle  it  awakens  in  me  a  peculiar  kind  of 
intellectual  pleasure,  because  it  bares  to  the  bodily  eye,  what  the  mind's  eye  grasps 
only  by  the  help  of  a  long  series  of  complicated  conclusions  for  the  waves  of  the 
invisible  atmospheric  ocean. 

We  have  to  imagine  a  perfectly  similar  spectacle  proceeding  in  the  interior  of  a 
baU-room,  for  instance.  Here  we  have  a  number  of  musical  instruments  in  action* 
speaking  men  and  women,  rustling  garments,  gliding  feet,  clinking  glasses,  and  so 
on.  All  these  causes  give  rise  to  systems  of  waves,  which  dart  through  the  mass 
of  air  in  the  room,  are  reflected  from  its  walls,  return,  strike  the  opposite  wall,  are 
again  reflected,  and  so  on  till  they  die  out.  We  have  to  imagine  that  from  the 
mouths  of  men  and  from  the  deeper  musical  instruments  there  proceed  waves  of 
from  8  to  12  feet  in  length  [c  to  jP],  from  the  lips  of  the  women  waves  of  2  to  4 
feet  in  length  [c"  to  &],  from  the  rustling  of  the  dresses  a  fine  small  crumple  of 
wave,  and  so  on ;  in  short,  a  tumbled  entanglement  of  the  most  different  kinds  of 
motion,  complicated  beyond  conception. 

Digitized  by  VjOOQlC 


CHAP.  XI.  ALGEBRAICAL  ADDITION  OF  WAVES.  27 

And  jet,  as  the  ea«r  is  able  to  distinguish  all  the  separate  constitaent  parts  of 
this  confused  whole,  we  are  forced  to  conclude  that  all  these  different  systems  of 
wave  coexist  in  the  mass  of  air,  and  leave  one  another  mutually  undisturbed. 
Bat  how  is  it  possible  for  them  to  coexist,  since  every  individual  train  of  waves  has 
at  any  particular  point  in  the  mass  of  air  its  own  particular  degree  of  condensa- 
tion and  rarefaction,  which  determines  the  velocity  .of  the  particles  of  air  to  this 
side  or  that  ?  It  is  evident  that  at  each  point  in  the  mass  of  air,  at  each  instant 
of  time,  there  can  be  only  one  single  degree  of  condensation,  and  that  the  particles 
of  air  can  be  moving  with  only  one  single  determinate  kind  of  motion,  having  only 
one  single  determinate  amount  of  velocity,  and  passing  in  only  one  single  deter- 
minate direction. 

What  happens  imder  such  circumstances  is  seen  directly  by  the  eye  in  the 
waves  of  water.  If  where  the  water  shews  large  waves  we  throw  a  stone  in,  the 
waves  thus  caused  will,  so  to  speak,  cut  into  the  larger  moving  surface,  and  this  ^ 
sarface  will  be  partly  raised,  and  partly  depressed,  by  the  new  waves,  in  such  a 
way  that  the  fresh  crests  of  the  rings  wiU  rise  just  as  much  above,  and  the  troughs 
sink  just  as  much  below  the  curved  surfaces  of  the  previous  larger  waves,  as  they 
would  have  risen  above  or  sunk  below  the  horizontal  surface  of  calm  water. 
Hence  where  a  crest  of  the  smaller  system  of  rings  of  waves  comes  upon  a  crest 
of  the  greater  system  of  waves,  the  surface  of  the  water  is  raised  by  the  sum  of 
the  two  heights,  and  where  a  trough  of  the  former  coincides  with  a  trough  of  the 
latter,  the  aurfiace  is  depressed  by  the  sum  of  the  two  depths.  This  may  be 
expressed  more  briefly  if  we  consider  the  heights  of  the  crests  above  the  level  of 
the  surface  at  rest,  as  positive  magnitudes,  and  the  depths  of  the  troughs  as  negative 
magnitudes,  and  then  form  the  so-caUed  algebraical  sum  of  these  positive  and 
negative  magnitudes,  in  which  case,  as  is  well  known,  two  positive  magnitudes 
(heights  of  crests)  must  be  added,  and  similarly  for  two  negative  magnitudes  (depths 
of  troughs) ;  but  when  both  negative  and  positive  concur,  one  is  to  be  subtracted  H 
from  the  other.  Performing  the  addition  then  in  this  algebraical  sense,  we  can 
express  our  description  of  the  surface  of  the  water  on  which  two  systems  of  waves 
concur,  in  the  following  simple  manner  :  The  distance  of  the  surface  of  the  water 
at  any  point  from  its  position  of  rest  is  at  any  moment  equal  to  the  [algebraical] 
sum  of  the  distances  at  which  it  would  have  stood  had  each  wave  acted  separately 
at  the  same  place  and  at  tJie  same  time. 

The  eye  most  clearly ^and  easily  distinguishes  the  action  in  such  a  case  as  has 
been  just  adduced,  where  a  smaller  circular  system  of  waves  is  produced  on  a  large 
rectilinear  system,  because  the  two  systems  are  then  strongly  distinguished  from 
each  other  both  by  the  height  and  shape  of  the  waves.  But  with  a  little  attention 
the  eye  recognises  the  same  fact  even  when  the  two  systems  of  waves  have  but 
slightly  different  forms,  as  when,  for  example,  long  rectilinear  waves  advancing 
towards  the  shore  concur  with  those  reflected  from  it  in  a  slightly  different 
direction.  In  this  case  we  observe  those  well-known  comb-backed  waves  where  H 
the  crest  of  one  system  of  waves  is  heightened  at  some  points  by  the  crests  of  the 
other  system,  and  at  others  depressed  by  its  troughs.  The  multipHcity  of  forms 
is  here  "extremely  great,  and  any  attempt  to  describe  them  would  lead  us  too 
far.  The  attentive  observer  will  readily  comprehend  the  result  by  examining 
any  disturbed  surface  of  water,  without  further  description.  It  will  suffice  for  our 
purpose  if  the  first  example  has  given  the  reader  a  clear  conception  of  what  is 
meant  by  adding  waves  together,* 

Hence  although  the  surface  of  the  water  at  any  instant  of  time  can  assume 
only  one  single  form,  while  each  of  two  different  systems  of  waves  simultaneously 
attempts  to  impress  its  own  shape  upon  it,  we  are  able  to  suppose  in  the  above 

*^  The  velocities  and  displacements  of  the  addition  of  waves  as  is  spoken  of  in  the  text,  I 
particles  of  water  are  also  to  be  added  accord-  is  not  perfectly  correct,  unless  the  heights  of  1 
ing  to  the  law  of  the  so-called  parallelogram  the  waves  are  infinitely  small  in  comparison  I 
of  forces.     Strictly  sijeaking,  such  a  simple      with  their  lengths.  \ 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


28  ALGEBRAICAL  ADDITION  OP  WAVES.  part  i. 

sense  that  the  two  systems  coexist  and  are  superimposed,  by  considering  the 
actual  elevations  and  depressions  of  the  sur&ce  to  be  suitabW.  separated  into  two 
parts,  each  of  which  belongs  to  one  of  the  systems  alone. 

In  the  same  sense,  then,  there  is  also  a  superimposition  of  different  systems  of 
sound  in  the  air.  By  each  train  of  waves  of  sound,  the  density  of  the  air  and  the 
velocity  and  position  of  the  particles  of  air,  are  temporarily  altered.  There  are 
places  in  the  wave  of  sound  comparable  with  the  crests  of  the  waves  of  water,  in 
which  the  quantity  of  the  air  is  increased,  and  the  air,  not  having  free  space  to 
escape,  is  condensed;  and  other  places  in  the  mass  of  air,  comparable  to  the 
l^oughs  of  the  waves  of  water,  having  a  diminished  quantity  of  air,  and  hence 
diminished  density.  It  is  true  that  two  different  degrees  of  density,  produced  by 
two  different  systems  of  waves,  cannot  coexist  in  the  same  place  at  the  same  time  ; 
nevertheless  the  condensations  and  rarefactions  of  the  air  can  be  (algebraically) 

il  added,  exactly  as  the  elevations  and  depressions  of  the  surface  of  the  water  in  the 
former  case.  Where  two  condensations  are  added  we  obtain  increased  condensation, 
where  two  rarefactions  are  added  we  have  increased  rarefaction ;  while  a  concur- 
rence of  condensation  and  rarefaction  mutually,  in  whole  or  in  part,  destroy  or 
neutralise  each  other. 

The  displacements  of  the  particles  of  air  are  compounded  in  a  similar  manner. 
If  the  displacements  of  two  different  systems  of  waves  are  not  in  the  same  direc- 
tion, they  are  compounded  diagonally  ;  for  example,  if  one  system  would  drive  a 
particle  of  air  upwards,  and  another  to  the  right,  its  real  path  will  be  obliquely 
upwards  towards  the  right.  For  our  present  purpose  there  is  no  occasion  to  enter 
more  particularly  into  such  compositions  of  motion  in  different  directions.  We 
are  only  interested  in  the  effect  of  the  mass  of  air  upon  the  ear,  and  for  this  we 
are  only  concerned  with  the  motion  of  the  air  in  the  passages  of  the  ear.  Now  the 
passages  of  our  ear  are  so  narrow  in  comparison  with  the  length  of  the  waves  of 

H  sound,  that  we  need  only  consider  such  motions  of  the  air  as  are  parallel  to  the 
axis  of  the  passages,  and  hence  have  only  to  distinguish  displacements  of  the 
particles  of  air  outwards  and  inwards,  that  is  towards  the  outer  air  and  towards 
the  interior  of  the  ear.  For  the  magnitude  of  these  displacements  as  well  as  for 
their  velocities  with  which  the  particles  of  air  move  outwards  and  inwards,  the 
same  (algebraical)  addition  holds  good  as  for  the  crests  and  troughs  of  waves  of 
water. 

Hence,  when  several  sonorous  bodies  in  the  surrounding  atmosphere,  simul- 
taneously excite  different  systems  of  waves  of  sound,  the  changes  of  density  of  the 
air,  and  the  displacements  and  velocities  of  the  particles  of  the  air  within  the 
passages  of  the  ear,  are  each  equal  to  the  (algebraical)  sum  of  the  corresponding 
changes  of  density,  displacements,  and  velocities,  which  each  system  of  waves 
wotUd  have  separately  produced,  if  it  had  acted  independently  ;  *  and  in  this  sense 
we  can  say  that  all  the  separate  vibrations  which  separate  waves  of  sound  would 

%  have  produced,  coexist  undisturbed  at  the  same  time  within  the  passages  of  our  ear. 
After  having  thus  in  dnswer  to  the  first  question  explained  in  what  sense  it  is 
possible  for  several  different  systems  of  waves  to  coexist  on  the  same  surface  of 
water  or  within  the  same  mass  of  air,  we  proceed  to  determine  the  means  possessed 
by  our  organs  of  sense,  for  analysing  this  composite  whole  into  its  original  consti- 
tuents. 

I  have  already  observed  that  an  eye  which  surveys  an  extensive  and  disturbed 
surface  of  water,  easily  distinguishes  the  separate  systems  of  waves  from  each 
other  and  follows  their  motions.  The  eye  has  a  great  advantage  over  the  ear  in 
being  able  to  survey  a  large  extent  of  surface  at  the  same  moment.  Hence  the 
eye  readily  sees  whether  the  individual  waves  of  water  are  rectilinear  or  curved, 
and  whether  they  have  the  same  centre  of  curvature,  and  in  what  direction  they 

*  The  eame  is  true  for  the  whole  moss  of      according  to  the  law  of  the  parallelogram  of 
external  air,  if  only  the  addition  of  the  dis*      forces, 
placements    in  different    directions  is  made 

Digitized  by  VjOOQIC 


CHAP.  iL  EYE  AND  EAR  CONTEASTED.  29 

are  adTancing.  All  these  observations  assist  it  in  determining  whether  two  systems 
of  waves  are  connected  or  not,  and  hence  in  discovering  their  corresponding  parts. 
Moreover,  on  the  surface  of  the  water,  waves  of  unequal  length  advance  with 
unequal  velocities,  so  that  if  they  coincide  at  one  moment  to  such  a  degree  as  to 
be  difficult  to  distinguish,  at  the  next  instant  one  train  pushes  on  and  the  other 
lags  behind,  so  that  they  become  again  separately  visible.  In  this  way,  then,  the 
observer  is  greatly  assisted  in  referring  each  system  to  its  point  of  departure,  and 
in  keeping  it  distinctly  visible  during  its  further  course.  For  the  eye,  then,  two 
systems  of  waves  having  different  points  of  departure  can  never  coalesce;  for 
example,  such  as  arise  from  two  stones  thrown  into  the  water  at  different  points. 
If  in  any  one  place  the  rings  of  wave  coincide  so  closely  as  not  to  be  easily 
separable,  they  always  remain  separate  during  the  greater  part  of  their  extent. 
Hence  the  eye  could  not  be  easily  brought  to  confuse  a  compound  with  a  simple 
undulatory  motion.  Yet  this  is  precisely  what  the  ear  does  under  similar  circum-  ^ 
stances  when  it  separates  the  musical  tone  which  has  proceeded  from  a  single 
source  of  sound,  into  a  series  of  simple  partial  tones. 

But  the  ear  is  much  more  unfavourably  situated  in  relation  to  a  system  of  waves 
of  sound,  than  the  eye  for  a  system  of  waves  of  water.  The  ear  is  affected  only 
by  the  motion  of  that  mass  of  air  which  happens  to  be  in  the  immediate  neigh- 
bourhood of  its  tympanum  within  the  aural  passage.  Since  a  transverse  section 
of  the  aural  passage  is  comparatively  smaU  in  comparison  with  the  length  of  waves 
of  sound  (which  for  serviceable  musical  tones  varies  from  6  inches  to  32  feet),*  it 
eorresponds  to  a  single  point  of  the  mass  of  air  in  motion.  It  is  so  smaU  that 
distinctly  different  degrees  of  density  or  velocity  could  scarcely  occur  upon  it, 
because  the  positions  of  greatest  and  least  density,  of  greatest  positive  and  nega- 
tive velocity,  are  always  separated  by  half  the  length  of  a  wave.  The  ear  is 
therefore  in  nearly  the  same  condition  as  the  eye  would  be  if  it  looked  at  one  point 
of  the  surface  of  the  water  through  a  long  narrow  tube,  which  would  permit  of  ^ 
seeing  its  rising  and  fEkUing,  and  were  then  required  to  undertake  an  analysis 
of  the  compound  waves.  It  is  easily  seen  that  the  eye  would,  in  most  cases, 
completely  fail  in  the  solution  of  such  a  problem.  The  ear  is  not  in  a  condition 
to  discover  how  the  air  is  moving  at  distant  spots,  whether  the  waves  which  strike 
it  are  spherical  or  plane,  whether  they  interlock  in  one  or  more  circles,  or  in  what 
direction  they  are  advancing.  The  circumstances  on  which  the  eye  chiefly  depends  | 
for  forming  a  judgment,  are  all  absent  for  the  ear. 

If,  then,  notwithstanding  all  these  difficulties,  the  ear  is  capable  of  distin- 
guishing musical  tones  arising  from  different  sources — and  it  really  shews  a 
marvellous  readiness  in  so  doing — it  must  employ  means  and  possess  properties 
altogether  different  from  those  employed  or  possessed  by  the  eye.  But  whatever 
these  means  may  be — and  we  shall  endeavour  to  determine  them  hereafter — ^it 
is  clear  that  the  analysis  of  a  composite  mass  of  musical  tones  must  in  the  first 
place  be  closely  connected  with  some  determinate  properties  of  the  motion  of  the  f 
air,  capable  of  impressing  themselves  even  on  such  a  very  minute  mass  of  air  as 
that  contained  in  the  aural  passage.  If  the  motions  of  the  particles  of  air  in  this 
passage  are  the  same  on  two  different  occasions,  the  ear  will  receive  the  same 
sensation,  whatever  be  the  origin  of  those  motions,  whether  they  spring  from  one 
or  several  sources. 

We  have  already  explained  that  the  mass  of  air  which  sets  the  tympanic 
membrane  of  the  ear  in  motion,  so  far  as  the  magnitudes  here  considered  are 
concerned,  must  be  looked  upon  as  a  single  point  in  the  surrounding  atmosphere. 
Are  there,  then,  any  peculiarities  in  the  motion  of  a  single  particle  of  air  which 
would  differ  for  a  single  musical  tone,  and  for  a  combination  of  musical  tones  ? 
We  have  seen  that  for  each  single  musical  tone  there  is  a  corresponding  periodical 

*  [These  are  of  course  rather  more  than  flue  organ  pipes.  See  Chap.  V.  sect.  5,  and 
twice  the  length  of  the  corresponding  open      compare  p.  26d,— Translator,] 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


so 


COMPOSITION  OF.  SIMPLE   WAVES. 


PART   I. 


motion  of  the  aii^,  and  that  its  pitch  is  determined  by  the  length  of  the  periodic 
time,  but  that  the  kind  of  motion  during  any  one  single  period  is  perfectly  arbitrary, 
and  may  indeed  be  infinitely  various.  If  then  the  motion  of  the  air  lying  in  the 
aural  passage  is  not  periodic,  or  if  at  least  its  periodic  time  is  not  as  short  as  that 
of  an  audible  musical  tone,  this  fact  will  distinguish  it  from  any  motion  which 
belongs  to  a  musical  tone ;  it  must  belong  either  to  noises  or  to  several  simultaneous 
musical  tones.  Of  this  kind  are  really  the  greater  number  of  cases  where  the  dif- 
ferent musical  tones  have  been  only  accidentally  combined,  and  are  therefore  not 
designedly  framed  into  musical  chords;  nay,  even  where  orchestral  music  is  per- 
formed, the  method  of  tempered  tuning  which  at  present  prevails,  prevents  an 
accurate  fulfilment  of  the  conditions  under  which  alone  the  resulting  motion  of 
the  air  can  be  exactly  periodic.  Hence  in  the  greater  number  of  cases  a  want 
of  periodicity  in  the  motion  might  furnish  a  mark  for  distinguishing  the  presence 
^  of  a  composite  mass  of  musical  tones. 

But  a  composite  mass  of  musical  tones  may  also  give  rise  to  a  purely  periodic 
motion  of  tJie  air,  namely,  when  all  tfie  mtisical  tones  which  intermingle,  have 
pitch  numbers  which  are  all  midtiples  of  one  and  the  same  old  number,  or,  which 

Fm.  IX. 


B       e 


comes  to  the  same  thing,  when  all  tliese  musical  tones,  so  far  as  their  pitch  is 
concerned,  may  be  regarded  as  the  upper  partial  tones  of  the  same  prime  tone.  It 
^  was  mentioned  in  Chapter  I.  (p.  22a,  b)  that  the  pitch  numbers  of  the  upper  partial 
tones  are  multiples  of  the  pitch  number  of  the  prime  tone.  The  meaning  of  this 
rule  will  be  clear  from  a  particular  example.  The  curve  A,  fig.  11,  represents  a 
pendular  motion  in  the  manner  explained  in  Chapter  I.  (p.  216),  as  produced  in  the 
air  of  the  aural  passage  by  a  tuning-fork  in  action.  The  horizontal  lengths  in  the 
curves  of  fig.  11,  consequently  represent  the  passing  time,  and  the  vertical  heights 
the  corresponding  displacements  of  the  particles  of  air  in  the  aural  passage.  Now 
suppose  that  with  the  first  simple  tone  to  which  the  curve  A  corresponds,  there  is 
sounded  a  second  simple  tone,  represented  by  the  curve  B,  an  Octave  higher  than 
the  first.  This  condition  requires  that  two  vibrations  of  the  curve  B  should  be 
made  in  the  same  time  as  one  vibration  of  the  curve  A.  In  A,  the  sections  of  the 
curve  do3  and  h  8^  are  perfectly  equal  and  similar.  The  curve  B  is  also  divided 
into  equal  and  similar  sections  e  c  and  c  cj  by  the  points  e,  c,  cj.  We  could  cer- 
tainly halve  each  of  the  sections  e  c  and  c  c^,  and  thus  obtain  equal  and  similar 
sections,  each  of  which  would  then  correspond  to  a  single  period  of  B.     But  by 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


CHAP.  II.  COMPOSITION  OF  SIMPLE   WAVES.  31 

taking  sections  consisting  of  two  periods  of  B,  we  divide  >B  into  larger  sections, 
each  of  which  is  of  the  same  horizontal  length,  and  hence  corresponds  to  the  same 
duration  of  time,  as  the  sections  of  A. 

If,  then,  both  simple  tones  are  heard  at  once,  and  the  times  of  the  points  e  and 
do,  c  and  8,  e^  and  8,  coincide,  the  heights  of  the  portions  of  the  section  of  carve 
e  €  have  to  be  [algebraically]  added  to  heights  of  the  section  of  curve  do8,  and 
similarly  for  the  sections  c  C|  and  88|.  The  result  of  this  addition  is  shewn  in  tlie 
carve  0.  The  dotted  line  is  a  duplicate  of  the  section  d^fi  in  the  curve  A.  Its 
object  is  to  make  the  composition  of  the  two  sections  immediately  evident  to  the 
eye.  It  is  easily  seen  that  the  curve  C  in  every  place  rises  as  much  above  or  sinks 
as  much  below  the  curve  A,  as  the  curve  B  respectively  rises  above  or  sinks 
beneath  the  horizontal  line.  The  heights  of  the  curve  0  are  consequently,  in  ac- 
cordance with  the  rule  for  compounding  vibrations,  equal  to  the  [algebraical]  sum 
of  the  corresponding  heights  of  A  and  B.  Thus  the  perpendicular  C|  in  C  is  the  f 
sum  of  the  perpendiculars  a,  and  b,  in  A  and  B  ;  the  lower  part  of  this  perpen- 
dicular C],  from  the  straight  hne  up  to  the  dotted  curve,  is  equal  to  the  perpen- 
dicular a|,  and  the  upper  part,  from  the  dotted  to  the  continuous  curve,  is  equal  to 
the  perpendicular  bj.  On  the  other  hand,  the  height  of  the  perpendicular  Cq  is 
equal  to  the  height  aa  diminished  by  the  depth  of  the  fail  bj.  And  in  the  same 
way  all  other  points  in  the  curve  C  are  found.* 

It  is  evident  that  the  motion  represented  by  the  curve  C  is  also  periodic,  and 
that  its  periods  have  the  same  duration  as  those  of  A.  Thus  the  addition  of  the 
section  do^  of  A  and  e  c  of  B,  must  give  the  same  result  as  the  addition  of  tlie 
perfectly  equal  and  similar  sections  S  S^  and  c  ci,  and,  if  we  supposed  both  curves 
to  be  continued,  the  same  would  be  the  case  for  all  the  sections  into  which  they 
would  be  divided.  It  is  also  evident  that  equal  sections  of  both  curves  could  not 
continually  coincide  in  this  way  after  completing  the  addition,  unless  the  curves  thus 
added  could  be  also  separated  into  exactly  equal  and  similar  sections  of  the  same  ^ 
length,  as  is  the  case  in  fig.  1 1 ,  where  two  periods  of  B  last  as  long  or  have  the 
same  horizontal  length  as  one  of  A.  Now  the  horizontal  lengths  of  our  figure 
represent  time,  and  if  we  pass  from  the  curves  to  the  real  motions,  it  results  that 
the  motion  of  air  caused  by  the  composition  of  the  two  simple  tones,  A  and  B,  is 
also  periodic,  just  because  one  of  these  simple  tones  makes  exactly  twice  as  many 
vibrations  as  the  other  in  the  same  time. 

It  is  easily  seen  by  this  example  that  the  peculiar  form  of  the  two  curves  A 
and  B  has  notliing  to  do  with  the  fact  that  their  sum  C  is  also  a  periodic  curve. 
Whatever  be  the  form  of  A  and  B,  provided  that  each  can  be  separated  into  equal 
and  similar  sections  which  have  the  same  horizontal  lengths  as  the  equal  and 
similar  sections  of  the  other — no  matter  whether  these  sections  correspond  to  one 
or  two,  or  three  periods  of  the  individual  curves— then  any  one  section  of  the  cune 
A  compounded  with  any  one  section  of  the  curve  B,  will  always  give  a  section 
of  the  curve  C,  which  will  have  the  same  length,  and  will  be  precisely  equal  and  f 
similar  to  any  other  section  of  the  curve  C  obtained  by  compounding  any  other 
section  of  A  with  any  other  section  of  B. 

When  such  a  section  embraces  several  periods  of  the  corresponding  curve  (as  in 
fig.  II,  the  sections  e  c  and  c  ci  each  consist  of  two  periods  of  the  simple  tone  B,) 
then  the  pitch  of  this  second  tone  B,  is  that  of  an  upper  partial  tone  of  a  prime 
(as  tlie  simple  lone  A  in  fig.  11),  whose  period  has  the  length  of  that  principal 
section,  in  accordance  with  the  rule  above  cited. 

In  order  to  give  a  slight  conception  of  the  multiplicity  of  forms  producible  by 
comparatively  simple  compositions,  I  may  remark  that  the  compound  curve  would 

*  [Readers  not  ased  to  geometrical  con-  sponding  perpendiculars  in  A  and  6  in  proper 

stractions  are  strongly  recommended  to  trace  directions,  and  joining  the  extremities  of  the 

the  two  enrves  A  and  B,  and  to  construct  the  lengths  thus  found  by  a  curved  line.    In  this 

curve  C  from  them,  by  drawing  a  number  of  way  only  can  a  clear  conception  of  the  com- 

perpendienlars  to  a  straight  line,  and  then  position  of  vibrations  be  rendered  sufficiently 

MMkig  off  upon  them  the  lengths  of  the  corre-  familiar  for  subFeqnent  u»e.—TraiisJator.] 

Digitized  by  VjOOQlC 


32 


DIFFERENCE  OF  PHASE. 


PABT    I. 


receive  another  form  if  the  carves  B,  fig.  ii,  were  displaced  a  little  with  respect  to 
the  curve  A  before  the  addition  were  commenced.  Let  B  be  displaced  by  being 
slid  to  the  right  mitil  the  point  e  falls  under  dj  in  A,  and  the  composition  will  then 
give  the  curve  D  with  narrow  crests  and  broad  troughs,  both  sides  of  the  crest 
being,  however,  equally  steep ;  whereas  in  the  curve  0  one  side  is  steeper  than  the 
other.  If  we  displace  the  curve  B  still  more  by  sliding  it  to  the  right  till  e  fialls 
under  d,,  the  compound  curve  would  resemble  the  reflection  of  G  in  a  mirror : 
that  is,  it  would  have  the  same  form  as  C  reversed  as  to  right  and  left  ;  the  steeper 
inclination  which  in  C  lies  to  the  left  would  now  he  to  the  right.  Again,  if  we 
displace  B  till  e  fedls  under  dj  we  obtain  a  curve  similiar  to  D,  fig.  ii,  but  reversed 
as  to  up  and  down,  as  may  be  seen  by  holding  the  book  upside-down,  the  crests 
being  broad  and  the  troughs  narrow. 


Pro 


All  these  curves  with  their  various  transitional  forms  are  periodic  curves. 
Other  composite  periodic  curves  are  shewn  at  G,  D,  fig.  12  above,  where  they  are 
compounded  of  the  two  curves  A  and  B,  having  their  periods  in  the  ratio  of  i  to  3. 
The  dotted  curves  are  as  before  copies  of  the  first  complete  vibration  or  period 
of  the  curve  A,  in  order  that  the  reader  may  see  at  a  glance  that  the  compound 
curve  is  always  as  much  higher  or  lower  than  A,  as  B  is  higher  or  lower  than  the 
horizontal  Hne.  In  G,  the  curves  A  and  B  are  added  as  they  stand,  but  for  D  the 
curve  B  has  been  first  slid  half  a  wave's  length  to  the  right,  and  then  the  addition 
f  has  been  effected.  Both  forms  differ  £rom  each  other  and  firom  all  preceding  ones. 
G  has  broad  crests  and  broad  troughs,  D  narrow  crests  and  narrow  troughs. 

In  these  and  similar  cases  we  have  seen  that  the  compound  motion  is  per£actly 
and  regularly  p^odic,  that  is,  it  is  exactly  of  the  same  kind  as  if  it  proceeded 
from  a  single  musical  tone.  The  curves  compounded  in  these  examples  correspond 
to  the  motions  of  single  simple  tones.  Thus,  the  motions  shown  in  fig.  11  (on 
p.  30&,  c)  might  have  been  produced  by  two  tuning-forks,  of  which  one  sounded  an 
Octave  higher  than  the  other.  But  we  shall  hereafter  see  that  a  flute  by  itself 
when  gently  blown  is  sufficient  to  create  a  motion  of  the  air  corresponding  to  that 
shown  in  0  or  D  of  fig.  11.  The  motions  of  fig.  12  might  be  produced  by  two 
tuning-forks  of  which  one  sounded  the  twelfth  of  the  other.  Also  a  single  closed 
organ  pipe  of  the  narrower  kind  (the  stop  called  Quintaten^)  would  give  nearly  the 
same  motion  as  that  of  G  or  D  in  fig.  12. 


*  [The  names  of  the  stops  on    German 
organs  do  not  always  agree   with   those  on 


English  organs.    I  find  it  best,  therefore,  ncyt 
to  translate  them,  but  to  give  their  ezplaiui. 


Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


CHAP.  II.  ANALYSIS  INTO  SIMPLE  VIBRATIONS.  33 

Here,  then,  the  motion  of  the  air  in  the  aural  passage  has  no  property  by  which 
the  composite*  musical  tone  can  be  distinguished  from  the  single  musical  tone. 
If  the  ear  is  not  assisted  by  other  accidental  circumstances,  as  by  one  tuning-fork 
beginning  to  sound  before  the  other,  so  that  we  hear  them  struck,  or,  in  the  other 
case,  the  rustling  of  the  wind  against  the  mouthpiece  of  the^flute  or  lip  of  the 
organ  pipe,  it  has  no  means  of  deciding  whether  the  musical  tone  is  simple  or 
composite. 

Now,  in  what  relation  does  the  ear  stand  to  such  a  motion  of  the  air  ?  Does 
it  analyse  it,  or  does  it  not  ?  Experience  shews  us  that  when  two  tuning-forks,  an 
Octave  or  a  Twelfth  apart  in  pitch,  are  sounded  together,  the  ear  is  quite  able  to 
distinguish  their  simple  tones,  although  the  distinction  is  a  Httle  more  diMcult 
with  these  than  with  other  intervals.  But  if  the  ear  is  able  to  analyse  a  compo- 
site musical  tone  produced  by  two  tuning-forks,  it  cannot  but  be  in  a  condition  to 
carry  out  a  similar  analysis,  when  the  same  motion  of  the  air  is  produced  by  a  ^ 
single  flute  or  organ  pipe.  And  this  is  really  the  case.  The  single  musical  tone 
of  such  instruments,  proceeding  from  a  single  source,  is,  as  we  have  already  men- 
tioned, analysed  into  partial  simple  tones,  consisting  in  each  case  of  a  prime  tone, 
and  one  upper  partial  tone,  the  latter  being  different  in  the  two  cases. 

The  analysis  of  a  single  musical  tone  into  a  series  of  partial  tones  depends, 
then,  upon  the  same  property  of  the  ear  as  that  which  enables  it  to  distinguish 
different  musical  tones  from  each  other,  and  it  must  necessarily  effect  both  analyses 
by  a  rule  which  is  independent  of  the  fact  that  the  waves  of  sound  are  produced 
by  one  or  by  several  musical  instruments. 

The  rule  by  which  the  ear  proceeds  in  its  analysis  was  first  laid  down  as 
generally  true  by  G.  S.  Ohm.  Part  of  this  rule  has  been  already  enunciated  in 
the  last  chapter  (p.  23a),  where  it  was  stated  that  only  that  particular  motion  of 
the  air  which  we  have  denominated  a  simple  vibration^  for  which  the  vibrating 
particles  swing  backwards  and  forwards  according  to  the  law  of  pendular  motion,  ^ 
is  capable  of  exciting  in  the  ear  the  sensation  of  a  single  simple  tone.  Every 
motion  of  tlie  air,  then,  which  corresponds  to  a  composite  viass  of  musical  tones, 
is,  according  to  Ohm's  law,  capable  of  being  ayvaly^ed  into  a  sum  of  simple  pen- 
dular vibrations,  and  to  each  such  siwjle  simple  vibration  corresponds  a  simple 
tone,  sensible  to  the  ear,  and  liaving  a  pitch  determined  by  the  periodic  time  of  the 
corresponding  motion  of  the  air. 

The  proofs  of  the  correctness  of  this  law,  the  reasons  why,  of  all  vibrational 
forms,  only  that  one  which  we  have  called  a  simple  vibration  plays  such  an 
important  part,  must  be  left  for  Chapters  IV.  and  VI.  Our  present  business  is 
only  to  gain  a  clear  conception  of  what  the  rule  means. 

The  simple  vibrational  form  is  inalterable  and  always  the  same.  It  is  only  its 
amplitude  and  its  periodic  time  which  are  subject  to  change.  But  we  have  seen 
in  figs.  II  and  12  (p.  306  and  p.  326)  what  varied  forms  the  composition  of  only  two 
simple  vibrations  can  prodiice.  The  number  of  these  forms  might  be  greatly  in-  ^ 
creased,  even  without  introducing  fresh  simple  vibrations  of  different  periodic 
times,  by  merely  changing  the  proportions  which  the  heights  of  the  two  simple 

tions  from  E.  J.    Hopkins's   TJie  Organ,   its  in  other  cases,  •  a  pipe  for  sounding  the  Twelfth 

'  History  and  Construction,  1870,  pp.  444-448.  in  addition  to  the  fundamental  tone.'   It  seems 

In  this   case    Mr.   Hopkins,  following  other  to  be   proporly  the  English    stop    •  Twelfth, 

aathoritiea,  prints  the  word  '  quintato^i/   and  Octave  Quint,  Duodecitna,^  No.  6ii,p.  141   of 

deiines  it,  in  16  feet  tone,  as  *  double  stopi)ed  Hopkins.  —  2Va?w/ator.j 

diapason,  of  rather  small  scale,  producing  the  *  [The   reader  must   distinguish   between 

Twelfth  of  the  fondamental  sound,  as  well  as  single  and  simple  musical  tones.    A  single  tone 

the  ground-tone  itself,  that  is,  somiding  the  may  be  a  compound  tone  inasmuch  as  it  may 

16  and  s\  ^^*  tones,'  which  means  sounding  the  be  compounded  of  several  simple  musical  tones, 

notes  beginning  with  C,,  simultaneously  witn  the  but  it  is  single  because  it  is  produced  hy  one 

notes  beginning  with  Q,  which  is  called   the  sounding  body.    A  composite  musical  toue  is 

5^  foot  tone,  because  according  to  the  organ-  necessarily  compound,  but  it  is  called  composite 

makers'  theory  (not  practice)  the  length  of  the  because  it  is  made  up  of  tones  (simple  or  com- 

G  pipe  is  ^  of  the  length  of  the  C  pipe,  and  i^of  pound)  produced  by  several  sounding  bodies. — 

16185^.     |.See  p.  I5<i',  note  J.j     And  smiilar ly.  Translator.] 

Digitized  by  V^OOQ IC 


34  ANALYSIS  INTO  SIMPLE   VIBRATIONS.  paet  i. 

vibrational  curves  A  and  B  bear  to  each  other,  or  displacing  the  curve  B  by  other 
distances  to  the  right  or  left,  than  those  already  selected  in  the  figures.  By  these 
simplest  possible  examples  of  such  compositions,  the  reader  will  be  able  to  form 
some  idea  of  the  enormous  variety  of  forms  which  would  result  from  using  more 
than  two  simple  forms  of  vibration,  each  form  representing  an  upper  partial  tone 
of  the  same  prime,  and  hence,  on  addition,  always  producing  fresh  periodic  curves- 
We  should  be  able  to  make  the  heights  of  each  single  simple  vibrational  curve 
greater  or  smaller  at  pleasure,  and  displace  each  one  separately  by  any  amount  in 
respect  to  the  prime, — or,  in  physical  language,  we  sliould  be  able  to  alter  their 
amplitudes  and  the  difference  of  their  phases ;  and  each  such  alteration  of  ampli- 
tude and  difference  of  phase  in  each  one  of  the  simple  vibrations  would  produce  a  fresh 
change  in  the  resulting  composite  vibrational  form.  [See  App.  XX.  sect.  M.  No.  2.] 
The  multiplicity  of  vibrational  forms  which  can  be  thus  produced  by  the  corn- 
el position  of  simple  pendular  vibrations  is  not  merely  extraordinarily  great :  it  is  so 
great  that  it  cannot  be  greater.  The  French  mathematician  Fourier  has  proved 
the  correctness  of  a  mathematical  law,  which  in  reference  to  our  present  subject 
may  be  thus  enunciated:  Any  given  regular  periodic  form  of  vibration  can 
always  be  produced  by  the  addition  of  simple  vibrations^  having  pitch  numbers 
which  are  once,  twice,  thrice,  four  times,  dc,  as  great  as  the  pitch  numbers  of  the 
given  motion. 

The  amplitudes  of  the  elementary  simple  vibrations  to  which  the  height  of  our 
wave-curves  corresponds,  and  the  difference  of  phase,  that  is,  the  relative  amount 
of  horizontal  displacement  of  the  wave-curves,  can  always  be  found  in  every  given 
case,  as  Fourier  has  shewn,  by  pecuhar  methods  of  calculation,  (wliich,  however, 
do  not  admit  of  any  popular  explanation,)  so  that  any  given  regularly  periodic 
motion  can  always  be  exhibited  in  one  single  way,  and  in  no  other  way  whatever^ 
as  the  sum  of  a  certain  number  of  pendular  vibrations, 
^  Since,  according  to  the  results  already  obtained,  any  regularly  periodic  motion 
corresponds  to  some  musical  tone,  and  any  simple  pendular  vibration  to  a  simple 
musical  tone,  these  propositions  of  Fourier  may  be  thus  expressed  in  acoustical 
terms : 

Any  vibrational  motion  of  the  air  in  the  entrance  to  the  ear,  correspondijig  to  a 
musical  tone,  may  be  always,  and  for  each  case  only  in  one  single  way,  exhibited  as 
the  sum  of  a  number  of  simple  vibratioyial  motions,  corresponding  to  the  partials 
of  this  musical  tone. 

Since,  according  to  these  propositions,  any  form  of  vibration,  no  matter  what 
shape  it  may  take,  can  be  expressed  as  the  sum  of  simple  vibrations,  its  analysis 
into  such  a  sum  is  quite  independent  of  the  power  of  the  eye  to  perceive,  by  looking 
at  its  representative  curve,  whether  it  contains  simple  vibrations  or  not,  and  if  it 
does,  what  they  are.  I  am  obUged  to  lay  stress  upon  this  point,  because  I  have  by 
no  means  unfrequently  found  even  physicists  start  on  the  false  hypothesis,  that  the 
^  vibrational  form  must  exhibit  little  waves  corresponding  to  the  several  audible 
upper  partial  tones.  A  mere  inspection  of  the  figs.  11  and  12  (p.  306  and  p.  326) 
will  snfiice  to  shew  that  although  the  composition  can  be  easily  traced  in  the  parts 
where  the  curve  of  the  prime  tone  is  dotted  in,  this  is  quite  impossible  in  those 
parts  of  the  curves  C  and  D  in  each  figure,  where  no  such  assistance  has  been 
provided.  Or,  if  we  suppose  that  an  observer  who  had  rendered  himself  thoroughly 
familiar  with  the  curves  of  simple  vibrations  imagined  that  he  could  trace  the  com- 
position in  these  easy  cases,  he  would  certainly  utterly  fail  on  attempting  to  dis- 
cover by  his  eye  alone  the  composition  of  such  curves  as  are  shewn  in  figs.  8 
and  9  (p.  21c).  In  these  will  be  found  straight  lines  and  acute  angles.  Perhaps 
it  will  be  asked  how  it  is  possible  by  compounding  such  smooth  and  uniformly 
rounded  curves  as  those  of  our  simple  vibrational  forms  A  and  B  in  figs.  1 1  and 
12,  to  generate  at  one  time  straight  lines,  and  at  another  acute  angles.  The 
answer  is,  that  an  infinite  number  of  simple  vibrations  are  required  to  generate 
curves  with  such  discontinuities  as  are  there  shewn.    But  when  a  great  many 

Digitized  by  V^OOQIC 


CHAP.n.  ANALYSIS  INTO  SIMPLE  VIBRATIONS.  35 

such  cnrves  are  combined,  and  are  so  chosen  that  in  certain  places  they  all  bend 
in  the  same  direction,  and  in  others  in  opposite  directions,  the  curvatures  mutually  1 

strengthen  each  other  in  the  first  case,  finally  producing  an  infinitely  great  curva- 
ture, that  is,  an  acute  angle,  and  in  the  second  case  they  mutually  weaken  each 
other,  80  that  ultimately  a  straight  line  results.  Hence  we  can  generally  lay  it 
down  as  a  rule  that  the  force  or  loudness  of  the  upper  partial  tones  is  the  greater, 
the  sharper  the  discontinuities  of  the  atmospheric  motion.  When  the  motion 
alters  uniformly  and  gradually,  answering  to  a  vibrational  curve  proceeding  in 
smoothly  curved  forms,  only  the  deeper  partial  tones,  which  lie  nearest  to  the 
prime  tone,  have  any  perceptible  intensity.  But  where  the  motion  alters  by  jumps, 
and  hence  the  vibrational  curves  shew  angles  or  sudden  changes  of  curvature,  the 
upper  partial  tones  will  also  have  sensible  force,  although  in  all  these  cases  the 
amplitudes  decrease  as  the  pitch  of  the  upper  partial  tones  becomes  higher.* 

We  shall  become  acquainted  with  examples  of  the  analysis  of  given  vibrational  ^ 
forms  into  separate  partial  tones  in  Chapter  V  .  ^' 

The  theorem  of  Fourier  here  adduced  shews  first  that  it  is  mathematically 
possible  to  consider  a  musical  tone  as  a  sum  of  simple  tones,  in  the  meaning  we 
have  attached  to  the  words,  and  mathematicians  have  indeed  always  found  it 
convenient  to  base  their  acoustic  investigations  on  this  mode  of  analysing  vibrations. 
But  it  by  no  means  follows  that  we  are  obliged  to  consider  the  matter  in  this  way. .  t«^>f**»^^** 
We  have  rather  to  inquire,  do  these  partial  constituents  of  a  musical  tone,  such  as  I 
the  mathematical  theory  distinguishes  and  the  ear  perceives,  really  exist  in  the  1 
mass  of  air  external  to  the  ear  ?  Is  this  means  of  analysing  forms  of  vibration  ' 
which  Fourier's  theorem  prescribes  and  renders  possible,  not  merely  a  mathematical 
fiction,  permissible  for  facihtating  calculation,  but  not  necessarily  having  any 
corresponding  actual  meaning  in  things  themselves  ?  What  makes  us  hit  upon 
pendular  vibrations,  and  none  other,  as  the  simplest  element  of  all  motions  pro- 
ducing sound  ?  We  can  conceive  a  whole  to  be  split  into  parts  in  very  different 
and  arbitrary  ways.  •  Thus  we  may  find  it  convenient  for  a  certain  calculation  to  ^ 
consider  the  number  12  as  the  sum  8-I-4,  because  the  8  may  have  to  be  cancelled, 
but  it  does  not  follow  that  12  must  always  and  necessarily  be  considered  as  merely 
the  sum  of  8  and  4.  In  another  case  it  might  be  more  convenient  to  consider  1 2 
as  the  sum  of  7  and  5.  Just  as  little  does  the  mathematical  possibility,  proved  by 
Fourier,  of  compounding  all  periodic  vibrations  out  of  simple  vibrations,  justify 
us  in  concluding  that  this  is  the  only  permissible  form  of  analysis,  if  we  cannot  in 
addition  establish  that  jhis  analysis  has^jjag  jan-^asential  meaning^in  nature.  That 
this  is  indeed  the  case,  that  this  analysis  has  a  meaning  in  nature  independently 
of  theory,  is  rendered  probable  by  the  fact  that  the  ear  really  effects  the  same 
anal^B,  and  also  by^the  circumstance  already  named,  that  this  kind  of  analysis 
has  been  Tound  so  mucb,.more  advantageous  in  mathematical  investigations' than 
^y^other.  Those  modes  of  regarding  phenomena  that  correspond  to  the  most 
intimate  constitution  of  the  mjktter  under  investigation  are,  of  course,  also  always 
fcHoie  which  lead  to  the  most  suitable  and  evident  theoretical  treatment.  But  it 
woiJd  notlbe  ad^'isable  to  begin  the  investigation  with  the  functions  of  the  ear, 
because  these  are  very  intricate,  and  in  themselves  require  much  explanation. 
In  the  next  chapter,  therefore,  we  shall  inquire  whether  the  analysis  of  compound 
into  simple  vibrations  has  an  actually  sensible,  meaning  in  the  external  world, 
independently  of  the  action  of  the  ear,  and  we  shall  really  be  in  a  condition  to 
shew  that  certain  mechanical  effects  depend  upon  whether  a  certain  partial  tone 

*  SupjKjsing  n  to  be  the  number  of  the      a  sudden  jump,  and  hence  the  curve  has  an 

order  of  a  partial  tone,  and  n  to  be  very  large,       „«„♦«  ««„i«.  ^\  „„      '        „,v. *i,  x 

Ax^      >i_  i«x  J      t \\  A-  1  *  acute  angle;  3)  as  ,  when  the  curvature 

then  the  amplitude  of  the  upper  partial  tones  ^       '^'       n.n.n 

decreases:  i)  as  A,  when  the  amplitude  of  the  "-^f"  suddenly ;  4)  when  none  of  the  diflferen- 

n  tial  quotients   are  discontinuous,   they  must 

vibrations  themselves  makes  a  sudden  jump;  ^^^^.^^^33  ^t  least  as  fast  as  e'^ 
2)  as  —  -,  when  their  differential  quotient  makes 


•^  D  2 

Digitized  by 


Google 


36  MECHANICS  OF   SYMPATHETIC  KESONANCE.  pabt  i. 

is  or  is  not  contained  in  a  composite  mass  of  musical  tones.  The  existence 
of  partial  tones  will  tbus  acquire  a  meaning  in  nature,  and  our^kno^jSge  of 
their  mechanical  effects  wilTin  turn  shed  a  new  light  on  their  relations  to  the 
Buman  ear. 


CHAPTER  III. 

ANALYSIS   OF   MUSICAL   TONES   BY   SYMPATHETIC   RESONANCE. 

We  proceed  to  shew  that  the  simple  partial  tones  contained  in  a  composite  mass 
of  musical  tones,  produce  peculiar  mechanical  effects  in  nature,  altogether  inde- 
pendent of  the  human  ear  and  its  sensations,  and  also  altogether  independent  of 
%  merely  theoretical  considerations.  These  effects  consequently  give  a  peculiar  objec- 
tive significance  to  this  peculiar  method  of  analysing  vibrational  forms. 

Such  an  effect  occurs  in  the  phenomenon  of  sympathetic  resonance.  This 
phpnomenon  is  always  found  in  those  bodies  which  when  once  set  in  motion  by 
any  impulse,  continue  to  perform  a  long  series  of  vibrations  before  they  come  to 
rest.  When  these  bodies  are  struck  gently,  but  periodically,  although  each  blow 
may  be  separately  quite  insufficient  to  produce  a  sensible  motion  in  the  vibratory 
body,  yet,  provided  the  periodic  time  of  the  gentle  blows  is  precisely  the  same  as 
the  periodic  time  of  the  body's  ovm  vibrations,  very  large  and  powerful  oscilla- 
tions may  result.  But  if  the  periodic  time  of  the  regular  blows  is  different  from 
the  periodic  time  of  the  oscillations,  the  resulting  motion  will  be  weak  or  quite 
insensible. 

Periodic  impulses  of  this  kind  generally  proceed  from  another  body  which  is 
already  vibrating  regularly,  and  in  this  case  the  swings  of  the  latter  in  the  course 
f  of  a  little  time,  call  into  action  the  swings  of  the  former.  Under  these  circum- 
stances we  have  the  process  called  sympathetic  oscillation  or  sympathetic  resonayice. 
The  essence  of  the  mechanical  effect  is  independent  of  the  rate  of  motion,  which 
may  be  fast  enough  to  excite  the  sensation  of  sound,  or  slow  enough  not  to  produce 
anything  of  the  kind.  Musicians  are  well  acquainted  with  sympathetic  resonance. 
When,  for  example,  the  strings  of  two  violins  are  in  exsrct  unison,  and  one  string  is 
bowed,  the  other  will  begin  to  vibrate.  But  the  nature  of  the  process  is  best  seen 
in  instances  where  the  vibrations  are  slow  enough  for  the  eye  to  follow  the  whole 
of  their  successive  phases. 

Thus,  for  example,  it  is  known  that  the  largest  church-bells  may  be  set  in  motion 
by  a  man,  or  even  a  boy,  who  pulls  the  ropes  attached  to  them  at  proper  and  regular 
intervals,  even  when  their  weight  of  metal  is  so  great  that  the  strongest  man  could 
scarcely  move  them  sensibly,  if  he  did  not  apply  his  strength  in  determinate 
periodical  intervals.  When  such  a  bell  is  once  set  in  motion,  it  continues,  like  a 
f  struck  pendulum,  to  oscillate  for  some  time,  until  it  gradually  returns  to  rest,  even 
if  it  is  left  quite  by  itself,  and  no  force  is  employed  to  arrest  its  motion.  The 
motion  diminishes  gradually,  as  we  know,  because  the  friction  on  the  axis  and  the 
resistance  of  the  air  at  every  swing  destroy  a  portion  of  tlie  existing  moving  force. 
As  the  bell  swings  backwards  and  forwards,  the  lever  and  rope  fixed  to  its  axis 
rise  and  fall.  If  when  the  lever  falls  a  boy  clings  to  the  lower  end  of  the  bell-rope, 
his  weight  will  act  so  as  to  increase  the  rapidity  of  the  existing  motion.  This 
increase  of  velocity  may  be  very  small,  and  yet  it  will  produce  a  corresponding 
increase  in  the  extent  of  the  bell's  swings,  which  again  will  continue  for  a  while, 
until  destroyed  by  the  friction  and  resistance  of  the  air.  But  if  the  boy  clung  to  the 
bell-rope  at  a  wrong  time,  while  it  was  ascending,  for  instance,  the  weight  of  his 
body  would  act  in  opposition  to  the  motion  of  the  bell,  and  the  extent  of  swing 
would  decrease.  Now,  if  the  boy  continued  to  cling  to  the  rope  at  each  swing  so 
long  as  it  was  falling,  and  then  let  it  ascend  freely,  at  every  avdng  the  motion  of 
the  bell  would  be  only  increased  in  speed,  and  its  swings  would  gi-adually  become 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQIC 


CHAP.  ra.      MECHANICS  OF  SYMPATHETIC  RESONANCE.  37 

greater  and  greater,  until  by  their  increase  the  motion  imparted  on  every  oscillation 
of  the  bell  to  the  walls  of  the  belfry,  and  the  external  air  would  become  so  great 
as  exactly  to  be  covered  by  the  power  exerted  by  the  boy  at  each  swing. 

The  success  of  this  process  depends,  therefore,  essentially  on  the  boy's  applying 
his  force  only  at  those  moments  when  it  will  increase  the  motion  of  the  bell.  That 
is,  he  must  employ  his  strength  periodically,  and  the  periodic  time  must  be  equal 
to  that  of  the  bell's  swing,  or  he  will  not  be  successful.  He  would  just  as  easily 
bring  the  swinging  bell  to  rest,  if  he  clung  to  the  rope  only  during  its  ascent,  and 
thus  let  his  weight  be  raised  by  the  bell. 

A  similar  experiment  which  can  be  tried  at  any  instant  is  the  following.  Con- 
struct a  pendulum  by  hanging  a  heavy  body  (such  as  a  ring)  to  the  lower  end  of  a 
thread,  holding  the  upper  end  in  the  hand.  On  setting  the  ring  into  gentle  pen- 
dular  vibration,  it  will  be  found  that  this  motion  can  be  gradually  and  considerably 
increased  by  watching  the  moment  when  the  pendulum  has  reached  its  greatest  IT 
departure  from  the  vertical,  and  then  giving  the  hand  a  very  small  motion  in  the 
opposite  direction.  Thus,  when  the  pendulum  is  furthest  to  the  right,  move  the 
hand  very  slightly  to  the  left ;  and  when  the  pendulum  is  furthest  to  the  left,  move 
the  hand  to  the  right.  The  pendulum  may  be  also  set  in  motion  from  a  state  of 
rest  by  giving  the  hand  similar  very  slight  motions  having  the  same  periodic  time 
as  the  penduhma's  own  swings.  The  displacements  of  the  hand  may  be  so  small 
under  these  circumstances,  that  they  can  scarcely  be  perceived  with  the  closest 
attention,  a  circumstance  to  which  is  due  the  superstitious  application  of  this 
httle  apparatus  as  a  divining  rod.  If  namely  the  observer,  without  thinking  of 
his  hand,  follows  the  swings  of  the  pendulum  with  his  eye,  the  hand  readily  follows 
the  eye,  and  involuntarily  moves  a  httle  backwards  or  forwards,  precisely  in  the 
same  time  as  the  pendulum,  after  this  has  accidentally  begun  to  move.  These 
involuntary  motions  of  the  hand  are  usually  overlooked,  at  least  when  the  observer 
is  not  accustomed  to  exact  observations  on  such  unobtrusive  influences.  By  this  1| 
means  any  existing  vibration  of  the  pendulum  is  increased  and  kept  up,  and  any 
accidental  motion  of  the  ring  is  readily  converted '  into  pendular  vibrations, 
which  seem  to  arise  spontaneously  without  any  co-operation  of  the  observer, 
and  are  hence  attributed  to  the  influence  of  hidden  metals,  running  streams,  and 
so  on. 

K  on  the  other  hand  the  motion  of  the  hand  is  intentionally  made  in  the  con- 
trary direction,  the  pendulum  soon  comes  to  rest. 

The  explanation  of  the  process  is  very  simple.  When  the  upper  end  of  the 
thread  is  fastened  to  an  immovable  support,  the  pendulum,  once  struck,  continues 
to  swing  for  a  long  time,  and  the  extent  of  its  swings  diminishes  very  slowly.  We 
can  suppose  the  extent  of  the  swings  to  be  measured  by  the  angle  which  the  thread 
makes  with  the  vertical  on  its  greatest  deflection  from  it.  If  the  attached  body 
at  the  point  of  greatest  deflection  lies  to  the  right,  and  we  move  the  hand  to  the 
left,  we  manifestly  increase  the  angle  between  the  string  and  the  vertical,  and  con-  ^F 
fiequently  also  augment  the  extent  of  the  swing.  By  moving  the  upper  end  of  the 
string  in  the  opposite  direction  we  should  decrease  the  extent  of  the  swing. 

In  this  case  there  is  no  necessity  for  moving  the  hand  in  the  same  periodic  time 
as  the  pendulum  swings.  We  might  move  the  hand  backwards  and  forwards  only 
at  every  third  or  fifth  or  other  swing  of  the  pendulum,  and  we  should  still  produce 
large  swings.  Thus,  when  the  pendulum  is  to  the  right,  move  the  hand  to  the 
left,  and  keep  it  still,  till  the  pendulum  has  swung  to  the  left,  then  again  to  the 
right,  and  then  once  more  to  the  left,  and  then  return  the  hand  to  its  first  position, 
afterwards  wait  till  the  pendulum  has  swung  to  the  riglil,  then  to  the  left,  and 
again  to  the  right,  and  then  recommence  the  first  motion  of  the  hand.  In  this 
way  three  complete  vibrations,  or  double  excursions  of  the  pendulum,  will  corre- 
spond to  one  left  and  right  motion  of  the  hand.  In  tlie  same  way  one  left  and 
right  motion  of  the  hand  may  be  made  to  correspond  with  seven  or  more  swings 
of  the  pendulum.     The  meaning  of  this  process  is  always  that  the  motion  of  the 

Digitized  by^OOQlC 


38  MECHANICS  OF   SYMPATHETIC  EESONANCE.  pabt  i. 

hand  must  in  each  case  be  made  at  such  a  time  and  in  such  a  direction  as  to  be 
opposed  to  the  deflection  of  the  pendulum  and  consequently  to  increase  it. 

By  a  slight  alteration  of  the  process  we  can  easily  make  two,  four,  six,  &c., 
swings  of  the  pendulum  correspond  to  one  left  and  right  motion  of  the  hand  ;  for 
a  sudden  motion  of  the  hand  at  the  instant  of  the  pendulum's  passage  through  the 
vertical  has  no  influence  on  the  size  of  the  swings.  Hence  when  the  pendulum 
lies  to  the  right  move  the  hand  to  the  left,  and  so  increase  its  velocity,  let  it  swing 
to  the  left,  watch  for  the  moment  of  its  passing  the  vertical  line,  and  at  that  instant 
return  the  hand  to  its  original  position,  allow  it  to  reach  the  right,  and  then  again 
the  left  and  once  more  the  right  extremity  of  its  arc,  and  then  recommence  the 
first  motion  of  the  hand. 

We  are  able  then  to  communicate  violent  motion  to  the  pendulum  by  very 
small  periodical  vibrations  of  the  hand,  having  their  periodic  time  exactly  as  great, 

%  or  else  two,  three,  four,  &c.,  times  as  great  as  that  of  the  pendular  oscillation.  We 
have  here  considered  that  the  motion  of  the  hand  is  backwards.  This  is  not 
necessary.  It  may  take  pla(5e  continuously  in  any  other  way  we  please.  When  it 
moves  continuously  there  will  be  generally  portions  of  time  during  which  it  will 
increase  the  pendulum's  motion,  and  others  perhaps  in  which  it  will  diminish  tlie 
same.  In  order  to  create  strong  vibrations  in  the  pendulum,  then,  it  will  be 
necessary  that  the  increments  of  motion  should  be  permanently  predominant,  and 
should  not  be  neutralised  by  the  sum  of  the  decrements. 

Now  if  a  determinate  periodic  motion  were  assigned  to  the  hand,  and  we  wished 
to  discover  whether  it  would  produce  considerable  vibrations  in  the  pendulum,  we 
could  not  always  predict  the  result  without  calculation.  Theoretical  mechanics 
would,  however,  prescribe  the  following  process  to  be  pursued :  Analyse  the  periodic 
motion  of  the  hand  into  a  sum  of  simple  peyidular  vibrations  of  the  Jiand — exactly 
in  the  same  way  as  was  laid  down  in  the  last  chapter  for  the  periodic  motions  of 

If  the  particles  of  air, — then,  if  the  periodic  tims  of  one  of  these  vibrations  is  equal 
to  the  periodic  time  of  tJie  pendulum's  own  oscillations,  the  pendulum  will  be  set 
ifito  violent  motion,  but  not  otherwise.  We  might  compound  small  pendular 
motions  of  the  hand  out  of  vibrations  of  other  periodic  times,  as  much  as  we  liked, 
but  we  should  fail  to  produce  any  lasting  strong  swings  of  the  pendulum.  Hence 
the  analysis  of  the  motion  of  the  hand  into  pendular  swings  has  a  real  meaning  in 
nature,  producing  determinate  mechanical  effects,  and  for  the  present  purpose  no 
other  analysis  of  the  motion  of  the  hand  into  any  other  partial  motions  can  be 
substituted  for  it. 

In  the  above  examples  the  pendulum  could  be  set  into  sympathetic  vibration, 
when  the  hand  moved  periodically  at  the  same  rate  as  the  pendulum ;  in  this  case 
the  longest  partial  vibration  of  the  hand,  corresponding  to  the  prime  tone  of  a 
resonant  vibration,  was,  so  to  speak,  in  unison  with  the  pendulum.  When  three 
swings  of  the  pendulum  went  to  one  backwards  and  forwards  motion  of  the  hand, 

IF  it  was  the  third  partial  swing  of  the  hand,  answering  as  it  were  to  the  Twelfth  of 
its  prime  tone,  which  set  the  pendulum  in  motion.     And  so  on. 

The  same  process  that  we  have  thus  become  acquainted  with  for  swings  of  long 
periodic  time,  holds  precisely  for  swings  of  so  short  a  period  as  sonorous  vibrations. 
Any  elastic  body  which  is  so  fastened  as  to  admit  of  continuing  its  vibrations  for 
some  length  of  time  when  once  set  in  motion,  can  also  be  made  to  vibrate  sym- 
pathetically, when  it  receives  periodic  agitations  of  comparatively  small  amounts, 
ha\dng  a  periodic  time  corresponding  to  that  of  its  own  tone. 

Gently  touch  one  of  the  keys  of  a  pianoforte  without  striking  the  string,  so  as 
to  raise  the  damper  only,  and  then  sing  a  note  of  the  corresponding  pitch  forcibly 
dh-ecting  the  voice  against  the  strings  of  the  instrument.  On  ceasing  to  sing,  the 
note  will  be  echoed  back  from  the  piano.  It  is  easy  to  discover  that  this  echo  is 
caused  by  the  string  which  is  in  unison  with  the  note,  for  directly  the  hand  is 
removed  from  the  key,  and  the  damper  is  allowed  to  fall,  the  echo  ceases.  The 
sympatlietic  vibration  of  the  string  is  still  better  shown  by  putting  little  paper 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


CHAP.m.  DIFFERENT  EXTENT  OF  SYMPATHETIC  RESONANCE.    39 

riders  upon  it,  which  are  jerked  off  as  soon  as  the  string  vibrates.  The  more 
exactly  the  singer  hits  the  pitch  of  the  string,  the  more  strongly  it  vibrates.  A 
very  httle  deviation  from  the  exact  pitch  fails  in  exciting  sympathetic  vibration. 

In  this  experiment  the  somiding  board  of  the  instrument  is  first  struck  by  the 
vibrations  of  the  air  excited  by  the  human  voice.  The  sounding  board  is  well 
known  to  consist  of  a  broad  flexible  wooden  plate,  which,  owing  to  its  exten- 
sive surface,  is  better  adapted  to  convey  the  agitation  of  the  strings  to  the  air, 
and  of  the  air  to  the  strings,  than  the  small  surface  over  which  string  and  air  are 
themselves  directly  in  contact.  The  sounding  board  first  communicates  the  agita- 
tions which  it  receives  from  the  air  excited  by  the  singer,  to  the  points  where  the 
string  is  fastened.  The  magnitude  of  any  single  such  agitation  is  of  course  infini- 
tesimally  small.  A  very  large  number  of  such  effects  must  necessarily  be  aggre- 
gated, before  any  sensible  motion  of  the  string  can  be  caused.  And  such  a  con- 
tinuous addition  of  effects  really  takes  place,  if,  as  in  the  preceding  experiments  with  ^ 
the  bell  and  the  pendulum,  the  periodic  time  of  the  small  agitations  which  are  com- 
municated to  the  extremities  of  the  string  by  the  air,  through  the  intervention  of  the 
sounding  board,  exactly  corresponds  to  the  periodic  time  of  the  string*s  Own  vibra- 
tions. When  this  is  the  case,  a  long  series  of  such  vibrations  will  really  set  the 
string  into  motion  which  is  very  violent  in  comparison  with  the  exciting  cause. 

In  place  of  the  human  voice  we  might  of  course  use  any  other  musical  instru- 
ment. Provided  only  that  it  can  produce  the  tone  of  the  pianoforte  string  accu- 
rately and  sustain  it  powerfully,  it  will  bring  the  latter  into  sympathetic  vibration. 
In  place  of  a  pianoforte,  again,  we  can  employ  any  other  stringed  instrument 
having  a  sounding  board,  as  a  violin,  guitar,  harp,  &c.,  and  also  stretched  mem- 
branes, bells,  elastic  tongues  or  plates,  &c.,  provided  only  that  the  latter  are  so 
fastened  as  to  admit  of  their  giving  a  tone  of  sensible  duration  when  once  made 
to  sound. 

When  the  pitch  of  the  original  sounding  body  is  not  exactly  that  of  the  sym-  % 
pathising  body,  or  that  which  is  meant  to  vibrate  in  sympathy  with  it,  the  latter 
will  nevertheless  often  make  sensible  sympathetic  vibrations,  which  will  diminish 
in  amplitude  as  the  difference  of  pitch  increases.  But  in  this  respect  different 
sounding  bodies  shew  great  differences,  according  to  the  length  of  time  for  which 
they  continue  to  sound  after  having  been  set  in  action  before  communicating  their 
whole  motion  to  the  air. 

Bodies  of  small  mass,  which  readily  communicate  their  motion  to  the  air,  and 
quickly  cease  to  sound,  as,  for  example,  stretched  membranes,  or  violin  strings,  are 
readily  set  in  sympathetic  vibration,  because  the  motion  of  tlie  air  is  conversely 
readily  transferred  to  them,  and  they  are  also  sensibly  moved  by  sufficiently  strong 
agitations  of  the  air,  even  when  the  latter  have  not  precisely  the  same  periodic 
time  as  the  natural  tone  of  the  sympathising  bodies.  The  limits  of  pitch  capable 
of  exciting  sympathetic  vibration  are  consequently  a  little  wider  in  this  case.  By 
the  comparatively  greater  influence  of  the  motion  of  the  air  upon  liglit  elasti(  ^ 
bodies  of  this  kind  which  offer  but  little  resistance,  their  natural  periodic  time  can 
be  slightly  altered,  and  adapted  to  that  of  the  exciting  tone.  Massive  elastic 
bodies,  on  the  other  hand,  which  are  not  readily  movable,  and  are  slow  in  com- 
municating their  sonorous  vibrations  to  the  air,  such  as  bells  and  plates,  and  con- 
tinue to  sound  for  a  long  time,  are  also  more  difficult  to  move  by  the  air.  A  much 
longer  addition  of  effects  is  required  for  this  purpose,  and  consequently  it  is  also 
necessary  to  hit  the  pitch  of  their  own  tone  with  much  greater  nicety,  in  order  to 
make  them  vibrate  sympathetically.  Still  it  is  well  known  that  bell-shaped  glasses 
can  be  put  into  violent  motion  by  singing  their  proper  tone  into  them  ;  indeed  it  is 
related  that  singers  with  very  powerful  and  pure  voices,  have  sometimes  been  able 
to  crack  them  by  the  agitation  thus  caused.  The  principal  difficulty  in  this  experi- 
ment is  in  hitting  the  pitch  with  sufficient  precision,  and  retaining  the  tone  at  that 
exact  pitch  for  a  sufficient  length  of  time. 

Tuning-forks  are  the  most  difficult  bodies  to  set  in  sympathetic  vibration.     To 

Digitized  by  VjOOQIC 


40  INFLUENCE  OF  PAETIALS  ON  SYMPATHETIC  EESONANCE.  part  i. 


effect  this  they  may  be  fastened  on  sounding  boxes  which  have  been  exactly  tuned  to 

their  tone,  as  shewn  in  fig.  13.     If  we  have  two  such  forks  of  exactly  the  same 

pitch,  and  excite  one  by  a  violin  bow, 

the  other  will  begin  to  vibrate  in  sym-  ^^^'  '3- 

pathy,  even  if  placed  at  the  further 

end  of  the  same  room,  and  it  will  con- 
tinue to  sound,  after  the  first  has  been 

damped.      The  astonishing  nature  of 

such  a  case  of  sympathetic  vibration 

will  appear,  if  we  merely  compare  the 

heavy  and  powerful  mass  of  steel  set 

in  motion,  with  the  Hght  yielding  mass 

of  air  which  produces  the  effect  by  such 
%  small  motive  powers  that  they  could 

not  stir  the  lightest  spring  which  was 

not  in  tune  with  the  fork.     With  such 

forks  the  time  required  to  set  them 

in  full  swing  by  sympathetic  action, 

is  also  of  sensible  duration,  and  the 

slightest  disagreement  in  pitch  is  sufficient  to  produce  a  sensible  diminution  in 

the  sympathetic  effect.     By  sticking  a  piece  of  wax  to  one  prong  of  the  second 

fork,  sufficient  to  make  it  vibrate  once  in  a  second  less  than  the  first — a  difference 

of  pitch  scarcely  sensible  to  the  finest  ear — tlie  sympathetic  vibration  wiU  be 

wholly  destroyed. 

After  having  thus  described  the  phenomenon  of   sympathetic  vibration  in 

general,  we  proceed  to  investigate  the  influence  exerted  in  sympathetic  resonance 

by  the  different  forms  of  wave  of  a  musical  tone. 
%        First,  it  must  be  observed  that  most  elastic  bodies  which  have  been  set  into 

sustained  vibration  by  a  gentle  force  acting  periodically,  are  (with  a  few  exceptions 


1^02^^ 


Fio.  14. 


to  be  considered  hereafter)  always  made  to  swing  in  pendular  vibrations.  But  they 
are  in  general  capable  of  executing  several  kinds  of  such  vibration,  with  difierent 
periodic  times  and  with  a  different  distribution  over  the  various  parts  of  the 
vibrating  body.  Hence  to  the  different  lengths  of  the  periodic  times  correspond 
different  simple  tones  producible  on  such  an  elastic  body.  These  are  its  so-called 
proper  tones.  It  is,  however,  only  exceptionally,  as  in  strings  and  tlie  narrower 
kinds  of  organ  pipes,  that  these  proper  tones  correspond  in  pitch  with  the  har- 

Digitized  by  V^ O OQ IC 


CHAP,  in  INFLUENCE  OF  PAETIALS  ON  SYMPATHETIC  RESONANCE.  41 

monic  upper  partial  tones  of  a  musical  tone  already  mentioned.     They  are  for  the 
most  part  inharmonic  in  relation  to  the  prime  tone. 

In  many  cases  the  vibrations  and  their  mode  of  distribution  over  the  vibrating 
bodies  can  be  rendered  visible  by  strewing  a  little  fine  sand  over  the  latter.  Take,  for 
example,  a  membrane  (as  a  bladder  or  piece  of  thin  india-rubber)  stretched  over  a 
circular  ring.  In  fig.  14  are  shewn  the  various  forms  which  a  membrane  can 
assume  when  it  vibrates.  The  diameters  and  circles  on  the  surface  of  the  mem- 
brane, mark  those  points  which  remain  at  rest  during  the  vibration,  and  are  known 
as  nodal  lines.  By  these  the  surface  is  divided  into  a  number  of  compartments 
which  bend  alternately  up  and  down,  in  such  a  way  that  while  those  marked  ( -f  ) 
rise,  those  marked  (--)  fall.  Over  the  figures  a,  b,  c,  are  shewn  the  forms  of  a 
section  of  the  membrane  during  vibration.  Only  those  forms  of  motion  are  drawn 
which  correspond  with  the  deepest  and  most  easily  producible  tones  of  the  mem- 
brane. The  number  of  circles  and  diameters  can  be  increased  at  pleasure  by  ^ 
taking  a  sufficiently  thin  membrane,  and  stretching  it  with  sufficient  regularity, 
and  in  this  case  the  tones  would  continually  sharpen  in  pitch.  By  strewing  sand 
on  the  membrane  the  figures  are  easily  rendered  visible,  for  as  soon  as  it  begins 
to  vibrate  the  particles  of  sand  collect  on  the  nodal  lines. 

In  the  same  way  it  is  possible  to  render  visible  the  nodal  lines  and  forms  of 
vibration  of  oval  and  square  membranes,  and  of  differently-shaped  plane  elastic 
plates,  bars,  and  so  on.  These  form  a  series  of  very  interesting  phenomena  dis- 
covered by  Chladni,  but  to  pursue  them  would  lead  us  too  far  from  our  proper 
subject.  It  will  suffice  to  give  a  few  details  respecting  the  simplest  case,  that  of  a 
circular  membrane. 

In  the  time  required  by  the  membrane  to  execute  100  vibrations  of  the  form  a, 
fig.  14  (p.  40c),  the  number  of  vibrations  executed  by  the  other  forms  is  as 
follows : — 


Form  of  Vibration 


a  without  nodal  lines  . 

b  with  one  circle  .... 

c  with  two  circles 

d  with  one  diameter    . 

e  with  one  diameter  and  one  circle 

f  with  two  diameters  . 


Pitch  Number 


ICO 
229*6 

359-9 

159 

292 

214 


Cents* 


Notes  nearly 


1439 
2217 
805 
1858 
I3«7 


c 

d'  + 

b'b¥ 

ab 

dl  + 


The  prime  tone  has  been  here  arbitrarily  assumed  as  c,  in  order  to  note  the  inter- 
vals of  the  higher  tones.  Those  simple  tones  produced  by  the  membrane  which  are 
shghtly  higher  than  those  of  the  note  written,  are  marked  ( + ) ;  those  lower,  by 
( — ).  In  this  case  there  is  no  commensurable  ratio  between  the  prime  tone  and 
the  other  tones,  that  is,  none  expressible  in  whole  numbers. 

Strew  a  very  thin  membrane  of  this  kind  with  sand,  and  sound  its  prime  tone 
strongly  in  its  neighbourhood ;  the  sand  will  be  driven  by  the  vibrations  towards  % 
the  edge,  where  it  collects.  On  producing  another  of  the  tones  of  the  membrane, 
the  sand  collects  in  the  corresponding  nodal  lines,  and  we  are  thus  easily  able  to 
determine  to  which  of  its  tones  the  membrane  has  responded.  A  singer  who 
knows  how  to  hit  the  tones  of  the  membrane  correctly,  can  thus  easily  make  the 


*  [Cents  are  hundredths  of  an  equal  Semi- 
tone, and  are  exceedingly  valuable  as  measures 
of  any,  especially  unusual,  musical  intervals. 
They  are  fully  explained,  and  the  method  of 
calculating  them  from  the  Interval  Batios  is 
given  in  App.  XX.  sect.  C.  Here  it  need  only 
be  said  that  the  number  of  hundreds  of  cents 
is  the  number  of  equals  that  is,  pianoforte 
Semitones  in  the  interval,  and  these  may  be 
counted  on  the  keys  of  any  piano,  while  the 
units  and  tens  shew  the  number  of  hundredths 
of  a  Semitone  in  excess.     Wlierevor  cents  are 


spoken  of  in  the  text,  (as  in  this  table),  they 
must  be  considered  as  additions  by  the  transla- 
tor. In  the  present  case,  they  give  the  inter- 
vals exactly,  and  not  roughly  as  in  the  column 
of  notes.  Thus,  1439  cents  is  sharper  than  14 
Semitones  above  c,  that  is,  sharper  than  d'  by 
39  hundredths  of  a  Semitone,  or  about  ^  of  a 
Semitone,  and  1858  is  flatter  than  19  Semitones 
above  c,  that  is,  flatter  than  g  by  42  hun- 
dredths of  a  Semitone,  or  nearly  ^  a  Semitone. 
—  Translator.] 


Digitized  by 


Google 


42  INFLUENCE  OF  PARTIALS  ON  SYMPATHETIC  RESONANCE,   part  i. 

sand  arrange  itself  at  pleasure  in  one  order  or  the  other,  by  singing  the  correspond- 
ing tones  powerfully  at  a  distance.  But  in  general  the  simpler  figures  of  the  deeper 
tones  are  more  easily  generated  than  the  complicated  figures  of  the  upper  tones. 
It  is  easiest  of  all  to  set  the  membrane  in  general  motion  by  sounding  its  prime 
tone,  and  hence  such  membranes  have  been  much  used  in  acoustics  to  prove  the 
existence  of  some  determinate  tone  in  some  determinate  spot  of  the  surrounding 
air.  It  is  most  suitable  for  this  purpose  to  connect  the  membrane  with  an  inclosed 
mass  of  air.  A,  fig.  1 5 ,  is  a  glass  bottle, 
having  an  open  mouth  a,  and  m  place 
of  its  bottom  b,  a  stretched  membrane, 
consisting  of  wet  pig's  bladder,  al- 
lowed to  dry  after  it  has  been  stretched 
and  fastened.     At    c  is    attached    a 

H  single  fibre  of  a  silk  cocoon,  bearing  a 
drop  of  sealing-wax,  and  hanging  down 
like  a  pendulum  against  the  membrane. 
As  soon  as  the  membrane  vibrates,  the  little  pendulum  is  violently  agitated.  Such 
a  pendulum  is  very  convenient  as  long  as  we  have  no  reason  to  apprehend  any  con- 
fusion of  the  prime  tone  of  the  membrane  with  any  other  of  its  proper  tones.  There 
is  no  scattering  of  sand,  and  the  apparatus  is  therefore  always  in  order.  But  to  decide 
with  certainty  what  tones  are  really  agitating  the  membrane,  we  must  after  all 
place  the  bottle  with  its  mouth  downwards  and  strew  sand  on  the  membrane. 
However,  when  the  bottle  is  of  the  right  size,  and  the  membrane  uniformly 
stretched  and  fastened,  it  is  only  the  prime  tone  of  the  membrane  (slightly  altered 
by  that  of  the  sympathetically  vibrating  mass  of  air  in  the  bottle)  which  is  easily 
excited.  This  prime  tone  can  be  made  deeper  by  increasing  the  size  of  the  mem- 
brane, or  the  volume  of  the  bottle,  or  by  diminishing  the  tension  of  the  membrane 

%  or  size  of  the  orifice  of  the  bottle. 

A  stretched  membrane  of  this  kind,  whether  it  is  or  is  not  attached  to  the  bot- 
tom of  a  bottle,  will  not  only  be  set  in  vibration  by  musical  tones  of  the  same  pitch 
as  its  own  proper  tone,  but  also  by  such  musical  tones  as  contain  the  proper  tone 
of  the  membrane  among  its  upper  partial  tones.  Generally,  given  a  number  of 
interlacing  waves,  to  discover  whether  the  membrane  will  vibrate  sympathetically, 
we  must  suppose  the  motion  of  the  air  at  the  given  place  to  be  mathematically 
analysed  into  a  sum  of  pendular  vibrations.  If  there  is  one  such  vibration  among 
them,  of  which  the  periodic  time  is  the  same  as  that  of  any  one  of  the  proper  tones 
of  the  membrane,  the  corresponding  vibrational  form  of  the  membrane  will  be  super- 
induced. But  if  there  are  none  such,  or  none  sufficiently  powerful,  the  membrane 
will  remain  at  rest. 

In  this  case,  then,  we  also  find  that  the  analysis  of  the  motion  of  the  air  into 
pendular  vibrations,  and  the  existence  of  certain  vibrations  of  this  kind,  are  dcci- 

^  sive  for  the  sympathetic  vibration  of  the  membrane,  and  for  this  purpose  no  other 
similar  analysis  of  the  motion  of  the  air  can  be  substituted  for  its  analysis  into 
pendular  vibrations.  The  pendular  vibrations  into  which  the  composite  motion  of 
the  air  can  be  analysed,  here  shew  themselves  capable  of  producing  mechanical 

I   effects  in  external  nature,  independently  of  the  ear,  and  independently  of  mathe- 

1    matical  theory.     Hence  the  statement  is  confirmed,  that  the  theoretical  view  which 

I    first  led  mathematicians  to  this   method  of  analysing  compoimd  vibrations,    is 

I    founded  in  the  nature  of  the  thing  itself. 

As  an  example  take  the  following  description  of  a  single  experiment : — 
A  bottle  of  the  shape  shewn  in  fig.  1 5  above  was  covered  with  a  thin  vulcan- 
ised india-rubber  membrane,  of  which  the  vibrating  surface  was  49  millimetres 
(i*93  inches)*  in  diameter,  the  bottle  being  140  millimetres  (5-51  inches)  high,  and 

♦  [As  10  inches  are  exactly  254  milh*metres  the  calculation  of  one  set  of  measures  from 
and  1 00  metres,  that  is,  1 00,000  millimetres  are  the  other.  Ilonpfhly  we  may  assume  25  mm. 
3tj37  inches,  it  is  easy  to  form  little  tables  for       to  be  i  inch.     But  whenever  dimensions  are 

Digitized  by  V^ O OQ IC 


CHAP,  m. 


EESONATOKS. 


43 


Fig.  i6  a. 


having  an  opening  at  the  brass  mouth  of  13  millimetres  {'51  inches)  in  diameter. 
When  blown  it  gave  /'Jl,  and  the  sand  heaped  itself  in  a  circle  near  the  edge  of  the 
membrane.  The  same  circle  resulted  from  my  giving  the  same  tone  f'%  on  an 
harmonium,  or  its  deeper  Octave /Jl,  or  the  deeper  Twelfth  B.  Both  F%  and  Z> 
gave  the  same  circle,  but  more  weakly.  Now  the  /'J  of  the  membrane  is  the  prime 
tone  of  the  harmonium  tone/'jl,  the  second  partial  tone  of/iJ,  the  third  of  -B,  the 
fourth  oiF%  and  fifth  of  D.*  All  these  notes  on  being  sounded  set  the  membrane 
in  the  motion  due  to  its  deepest  tone.  A  second  smaller  circle,  19  millimetres 
(•75  inches)  in  diameter  was  produced  on  the  membrane  by  h'  and  the  same  more 
faintly  by  6,  and  there  was  a  trace  of  it  for  the  deeper  Twelfth  e,  that  is,  for  simple 
tones  of  which  vibrational  numbers  were  \  and  \  that  of  5'.t 

Stretched  membranes  of  this  kind  are  very  convenient  for  these  and  similar 
experiments  on  the  partials  of  compound  tones.     They  have  the  great  advantage 

of  being  independent  of  the  ear,  but  they  f 
are  not  very  sensitive  for  the  fainter  simple 
tones.  Their  sensitiveness  is  far  inferior  to 
that  of  the  res'ondtors  which  I  have  intro- 
duced. These  are  hollow  spheres  of  glass 
or  metal,  or  tubes,  with  two  openings  as 
shewn  in  figs.  16  a  and  16  b.  One  opening 
(a)  has  sharp  edges,  the  other  (b)  is  funnel- 
shaped,  and  adapted  for  insertion  into  the 
ear.  This  smaller  end  I  usually  coat  with 
melted  sealing  wax,  and  when  the  wax  has 
cooled  down  enough  not  to  hurt  the  finger 
on  being  touched,  but  is  still  soft,  I  press  the  opening  into  the  entrance  of  my 
ear.  The  sealing  wax  thus  moulds  itself  to  the  shape  of  the  inner  surface  of  this 
opening,  and  when  I  subsequently  use  the  resonator,  it  fits  easily  and  is  air-tight.  % 
Such  an  instrument  is  very  like  the  resonance  bottle  already  described,  fig.  15 
„      ,  ^  (p.  42a) ,  for  which  the  observer's 

Pig.  i6b.  vr  t      /» 

own  tympanic  membrane  has 
been  made  to  replace  the  for- 
mer artificial  membrane. 

The  mass  of  air  in  a  reso- 
nator, together  with  that  in  the 
aural  passage,  and  vnth  the 
tympanic  membrane  or  drumskin  itself,  forms  an  elastic  system  which  is  capable 
of  vibrating  in  a  peculiar  manner,  and,  in  especial,  the  prime  tone  of  the  sphere, 
which  is  much  deeper  than  any  other  of  its  proper  tones,  can  be  set  into  very 
powerful  sjrmpathetic  vibration,  and  then  the  ear,  which  is  in  immediate  connec- 
tion with  the  air  inside  the  sphere,  perceives  this  augmented  tone  by  direct  action. 
If  we  stop  one  ear  (which  is  best  done  by  a  plug  of  sealing  wax  moulded  into  the  ^ 
form  of  the  entrance  of  the  ear), J  and  apply  a  resonator  to  the  other,  most  of  the 
tones  produced  in  the  surrounding  air  will  be  considerably  damped;  but  if  the 
proper  tone  of  the  resonator  is  sounded,  it  brays  into  the  ear  most  powerfully. 


given  in  the  text  in  mm.  (that  is,  millimetres), 
they  will  be  reduced  to  inches  and  decimals  of 
to  mch.~Translator,] 

*  [As  the  instrument  was  tempered,  we 
should  have,  approximately,  for /g  the  partials 
/t ,  /5  ,  <fcc. ;  for  B  the  partials  B,  6,  fU  ,  Ac ; 
tor  Ft  the  partials  F%,fU,cU,fU,  Ac. ;  and 
for  D  the  partials  D,  d,  o,  d\  fU ,  Ac.  To 
prevent  confusion  I  have  reduced  the  upper 
partials  of  the  text  to  ordinary  partials,  as 
suggested  in  p.  236',  note.— Tranalaior.] 

t  [Here  the  partials  of  b  arc  6,  b\  Ac,  and 
of  e  aro  e,  e\  ^,  Ac,  ao  that  both  b  and  e 
contain  b^,— Translator,] 


X  [For  ordinary  purposes  this  is  quite 
enough,  indeed  it  is  generally  unnecessary  to 
stop  the  other  ear  at  all.  But  for  such  experi- 
ments as  Mr.  Bosanquet  had  to  make  on  beats 
(see  App.  XX.  section  L.  art.  4,  b)  he  was 
obliged  to  use  a  jar  as  the  resonator,  conduct 
the  sound  from  it  through  first  a  glass  and 
then  an  elastic  tube  to  a  semicircular  metal  tube 
which  reached  from  ear  to  ear,  to  each  end  of 
which  a  tube  coated  with  india-rubber,  could  be 
screwed  into  the  ear.  By  this  means,  when 
proper  care  was  taken,  all  sound  but  that 
coming  from  the  resonance  jar  was  perfectly 
excluded. — TranslaforJ]  t 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQ IC 


44  RESONATORS.  part  i. 

Hence  any  one,  even  if  he  has  no  ear  for  music  or  is  quite  unpractised  in  detecting 
musical  sounds,  is  put  in  a  condition  to  pick  the  required  simple  tone,  even  if  com- 
paratively faint,  from  out  of  a  great  number  of  others.  The  proper  tone  of  the 
resonator  may  even  be  sometimes  heard  cropping  up  in  the  whistling  of  the  >*'ind, 
the  rattling  of  carriage  wheels,  the  splashing  of  water.  For  these  purposes  such 
resonators  are  incomparably  more  sensitive  than  tuned  membranes.  When  the 
simple  tone  to  be  observed  is  faint  in  comparison  with  those  which  a^scompany  it, 
it  is  of  advantage  to  alternately  apply  and  withdraw  the  resonator.  We  thus  easily 
feel  whether  the  proper  tone  of  the  resonator  begins  to  sound  when  the  instrument 
is  apphed,  whereas  a  uniform  continuous  tone  is  not  so  readily  perceived. 

A  properly  tuned  series  of  such  resonators  is  therefore  an  important  instrument 
for  experiments  in  which  individual  faint  tones  have  to  be  distinctly  heard,  although 
accompanied  by  others  which  are  strong,  as  in  observations  on  the  combinational 

%  and  upper  partial  tones,  and  a  senes  of  other  phenomena  to  be  hereafter  described 
relating  to  chords.  By  their  means  such  researches  can  be  carried  out  even  by 
ears  quite  untrained  in  musical  observation,  whereas  it  had  been  previously 
impossible  to  conduct  them  except  by  trained  musical  ears,  and  much  strained 
attention  properly  assisted.  These  tones  were  consequently  accessible  to  the 
observation  of  only  a  very  few  individuals  ;  and  indeed  a  large  number  of  physi- 
cists and  even  musicians  had  never  succeeded  in  distinguishing  them.  And  again 
even  the  trained  ear  is  now  able,  with  the  assistance  of  resonators,  to  caiTy  the 
analysis  of  a  mass  of  musical  tones  much  further  than  before.  Without  their  help, 
indeed,  I  should  scarcely  have  succeeded  in  making  the  observations  hereafter 
described,  with  so  much  precision  and  certainty,  as  I  have  been  enabled  to  attain 
at  present.* 

It  must  be  carefully  noted  that  the  ear  does  not  hear  tlie  required  tone  with 
augmented  force,  unless  that  tone  attains  a  considerable  intensity  within  the  mass 

^  of  air  inclosed  in  the  resonator.  Now  the  mathematical  theory  of  the  motion  of 
the  air  shews  that,  so  long  as  the  amplitude  of  the  \'ibration8  is  sufficiently  small, 
the  inclosed  air  will  execute  pendular  oscillations  of  the  same  periodic  time  as 
those  in  the  external  air,  and  none  other,  and  that  only  those  pendular  oscillations 
whose  periodic  time  corresponds  with  that  of  the  proper  tone  of  the  resonator, 
have  any  considerable  strength  ;  the  intensity  of  the  rest  diminishing  as  the  differ- 
ence of  their  pitch  from  that  of  the  proper  tone  increases.  All  this  is  independent 
of  the  connection  of  the  ear  and  resonator,  except  in  so  far  as  its  tympanic  mem- 
brane forms  one  of  the  inclosing  walls  of  the  mass  of  air.  Theoretically  this 
apparatus  does  not  differ  from  the  bottle  with  an  elastic  membrane,  in  fig.  15 
(p.  42a),  but  its  sensitiveness  is  amazingly  increased  by  using  the  drumskin  of  the  ear 
for  the  closing  membrane  of  the  bottle,  and  thus  bringing  it  in  direct  connection 
with  the  auditory  ner\'es  themselves.  Hence  we  cannot  obtam  a  powerful  tone  in 
the   resonator  except  when   an  analysis  of  the  motion  of  the  external  air  into 

^  pendular  vibrations,  would  shew  that  one  of  them  has  the  same  periodic  time  aa 
the  proper  tone  of  the  resonator.  Here  again  no  other  analysis  but  that  into 
pendular  vibrations  would  give  a  correct  result. 

It  is  easy  for  an  observer  to  convince  himself  of  the  above-named  properties  of 
resonators.  Apply  one  to  the  ear,  and  let  a  pffice  of  harmonised  music,  in  which 
the  proper  tone  of  the  resonator  frequently  occurs,  be  executed  by  any  instruments. 
As  often  as  this  tone  is  struck,  the  ear  te  which  the  instrument  is  held,  will  hear 
it  violently  contrast  with  all  the  other  tones  of  the  chord. 

This  proper  tone  will  also  often  be  heard,  but  more  weakly,  when  deeper 
musical  tones  occur,  and  on  investigation  we  find  that  in  such  cases  tones  have 
been  struck  which  include  the  proper  tone  of  the  resonator  among  their  upper 
partial  tones.  Such  deeper  musical  tones  are  called  the  harmonic  uncUr  tones  of 
the  resonator.  They  are  musical  tones  whose  periodic  time  is  exactly  2,  3,  4,  5, 
and  so  on,  times  as  great  as  that  of  the  resonator.  Thus  if  the  proper  tone  of 
*  See  Appendix  II.  for  tlie  measurefl  and  different  forms  of  these  Krsonators. 

Digitized  by  VjOOQlC 


CHAP.  III.  SYMPATHETIC  RESONANCE   OF   STRINGS.  45 

the  resonator  is  c",  it  will  be  heard  when  a  musical  instrument  sounds  c',  /,  c,  A\), 
F,  D,  (7,  and  so  on.*  In  this  case  the  resonator  is  made  to  sound  in  sympathy 
with  one  of  the  harmonic  upper  partial  tones  of  the  compound  musical  tone  which 
is  vibrating  in  the  external  air.  It  must,  however,  be  noted  that  by  no  means  all 
the  haimonic  upper  partial  tones  occur  in  the  compound  tones  of  every  instrument, 
and  that  they  have  very  different  degrees  of  intensity  in  different  instruments.  In 
the  musical  tones  of  vioHns,  pianofortes,  and  hannoniums,  the  first  five  or  six  are 
generally  very  distinctly  present.  A  more  detailed  account  of  the  upper  partial 
tones  of  strings  will  be  given  in  the  next  chapter.  On  the  harmonium  the  un- 
evenly numbered  partial  tones  (i,  3,  5,  &c.)  are  generally  stronger  than  the  evenly 
numbered  ones  (2,  4,  6,  &c.).  In  the  same  way,  the  upper  partial  tones  are  clearly 
heard  by  means  of  the  resonators  in  the  singing  tones  of  the  human  voice,  but 
differ  in  strength  for  the  different  vowels,  as  wiU  be  shewn  hereafter.  II 


Among  the  bodies  capable  of  strong  sympathetic  vibration  must  be  reclconed 
stretched  strings  which  are  connected  with  a  sounding  board,  as  on  the  pianoforte. 

The  principal  mark  of  distinction  between  strings  and  the  other  bodies  which 
vibrate  sympathetically,  is  that  different  vibrating  forms  of  strings  give  simple 
tones  corresponding  to  the  harvwiiic  upper  partial  tones  of  the  prime  tone,  whereas 
the  secondary  simple  tones  of  membranes,  bells,  rods,  &c.,  are  iV^harmonic  with  the 
prime  tone,  and  the  masses  of  air  in  resonators  have  generally  only  very  high 
upper  partial  tones,  also  chiefly  mharmonic  with  the  prime  tone,  and  not  capable 
of  being  much  reinforced  by  the  resonator. 

The  vibrations  of  strings  may  be  studied  either  on  elastic  chords  loosely 
stretched,  and  not  sonorous,  but  swinging  so  slowly  that  their  motion  may  be 
followed  with  the  hand  and  eye,  or  else  on  sonorous  strings,  as  those  of  the  piano- 
forte, guitar,  monochord,  or  violin.  Strings  of  the  first  kind  are  best  made  of  thin  ^ 
spirals  of  brass  wire,  six  to  ten  feet  in  length.  They  should  be  gently  stretched, 
and  both  ends  should  be  fastened.  A  string  of  this  construction  is  capable  of 
making  very  large  excursions  with  great  regularity,  which  are  easily  seen  by  a  large 
audience.  The  swings  are  excited  by  moving  the  string  regularly  backwards  and 
forwards  by  the  finger  near  to  one  of  its  extremities. 

A  string  maybe  first  made  to  vibrate  as  in  fig.  17,  a  (p.  466),  so  that  its  appear- 
ance when  displaced  from  its  position  of  rest  is  always  that  of  a  simple  half  wave. 
The  string  in  this  case  gives  a  single  simple  tone,  the  deepest  it  can  produce,  and 
no  other  harmonic  secondary  tones  are  audible. 

But  the  string  may  also  during  its  motion  assume  the  forms  fig.  17,  b,  c,  d. 
In  this  case  the  form  of  the  string  is  that  of  two,  three,  or  four  half  waves  of  a 
simple  wave-curve.  In  the  vibrational  form  b  the  string  produces  only  the  upper 
Octave  of  its  prime  tone,  in  the  form  c  the  Twelfth,  and  in  the  form  d  the  second 
Octave.  The  dotted  lines  shew  the  position  of  the  string  at  the  end  of  half  its  ^ 
periodic  time.  In  b  the  point  p  remains  at  rest,  in  c  two  points  y^  and  y^  remain 
at  rest,  in  d  three  points  S,,  h^,  3,.  These  points  are  called  nodes.  In  a  swingmg 
spiral  wire  the  nodes  are  readily  seen,  and  for  a  resonant  string  they  are  shewn  by 
httle  paper  riders,  which  are  jerke^ofi*  from  the  vibrating  parts  and  remain  sitting 
on  the  nodes.  When,  then,  the  string  is  divided  by  a  node  into  two  swinging 
sections,  it  produces  a  simple  tone  having  a  pitch  number  double  that  of  the  prime 


[The  c"  occurs   as  the  2nd,  3rd,   4th, 
6th,  7th,  8th    partials    of   these   notes. 

the   7th    being  rather  flat.    The  partials  are 
in  fact  :— 

d     c" 

f     f      c" 

c      d     f      d' 

A\)  a\>    et>    a'\)     d' 

F    f      d     f       a' 

D     d      a      d'      fl 

C     c      f      d       d 

a    d' 

f    b'\>    c".     Translator.] 

Digitized  by  CjOOQI 

46 


SYMPATHETIC  RESONANCE  OF  STRINGS. 


PABTL 


tone.    For  three  sections  the  pitch  number  is  tripled,  for  fonr  sections  quadrupled, 
and  so  on. 

To  bring  a  spiral  wire  into  these  different  forms  of  vibration,  we  move  it 
periodically  with  the  finger  near  one  extremity,  adopting  the  period  of  its  slowest 
swings  for  a,  twice  that  rate  for  b,  three  times  for  c,  and  four  times  for  d.  Or  else 
we  just  gently  touch  one  of  the  nodes  nearest  the  extremity  with  the  finger,  and  pluck 
the  string  half-way  between  this  node  and  the  nearest  end.  Hence  when  y^  in  c, 
or  S,  in  d,  is  kept  at  rest  by  the  finger,  we  pluck  the  string  at  c  The  other  nodes 
then  appear  when  the  vibration  commences. 


f 


For  a  sonorous  string  the  vibrational  forms  of  fig.  17  above  are  most  purely 
produced  by  applying  to  its  sounding  board  the  handle  of  a  tuning-fork  which  has 
been  struck  and  gives  the  simple  tone  corresponding  to  the  form  required.  K  only 
a  determinate  number  of  nodes  are  desired,  and  it  is  indifferent  whether  the  indi- 
vidual points  of  the  string  do  or  do  not  execute  simple  vibrations,  it  is  sufficient  to 
touch  the  string  very  gently  at  one  of  the  nodes  and  either  pluck  the  string  or  rub 
it  with  a  violin  bow.  By  touching  the  string  with  the  finger  all  those  simple  vibra- 
tions are  damped  which  have  no  node  at  that  point,  and  only  those  remain  which 
allow  the  string  to  be  at  rest  in  that  place. 

The  number  of  nodes  in  long  thin  strings  may  be  considerable.  They  cease  to 
be  formed  when  the  sections  which  lie  between  the  nodes  are  too  short  and  stiff  to 
^  be  capable  of  sonorous  vibration.  Very  fine  strings  consequently  give  a  greater 
number  of  higher  tones  than  thicker  ones.  On  the  violin  and  the  lower  pianoforte 
strings  it  is  not  very  difficult  to  produce  tones  with  10  sections  ;  but  with  extremely 
fine  wires  tones  with  16  or  20  sections  can  be  made  to  sound.  [Also  compare  p.  78^.] 

The  forms  of  vibration  here  spoken  of  are  those  in  which  each  point  of  the 
string  performs  pendular  oscillations.  Hence  these  motions  excite  in  the  ear  the 
sensation  of  only  a  single  simple  tone.  In  all  other  vibrational  forms  of  the 
strings,  the  oscillations  are  not  simply  pendular,  but  take  place  according  to  a  differ- 
ent and  more  compUcated  law.  This  is  always  the  case  when  the  string  is  plucked 
in  the  usual  way  with  the  finger  (as  for  guitar,  harp,  zither)  or  is  struck  with  a 
hammer  (as  on  the  pianoforte),  or  is  rubbed  with  a  violin  bow.  The  resulting  motions 
may  then  be  regarded  as  compounded  of  many  simple  vibrations,  which,  w^hen 
taken  separately,  correspond  to  those  in  fig.  17.  The  multiphcity  of  such  com- 
posite forms  of  motion  is  infinitely  great,  the  string  may  indeed  be  considered 
as  capable  of  assuming  any  given  form  (provided  we  confine  ourselves  in  all  cases 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


CHAP.m.  SYMPATHETIC  EESONANCE  OF  STKINGS.  47 

to  very  small  deviations  from  the  position  of  rest),  because,  according  to  what  was 
said  in  Chapter  11.,  any  given  form  of  wave  can  be  compounded  out  of  a  number 
of  simple  waves  such  as  those  indicated  in  fig.  17,  a,  b,  c,  d.  A  plucked,  struck, 
or  bowed  string  therefore  allows  a  great  number  of  harmonic  upper  partial  tones  to 
be  heard  at  the  same  time  as  the  prime  tone,  and  generally  the  number  increases 
with  the  thinness  of  the  string.  The  peculiar  tinkling  sound  of  very  fine  metallic 
strings,  is  clearly  due  to  these  very  high  secondary  tones.  It  is  easy  to  distinguish 
tbe  upper  simple  tones  up  to  the  sixteenth  by  means  of  resonators.  Beyond  the 
sixteenth  they  are  too  close  to  each  other  to  be  distinctly  separable  by  this  means. 

Hence  when  a  string  is  sympathetically  excited  by  a  musical  tone  in  its  neigh- 
bom-hood,  answering  to  the  pitch  of  the  prime  tone  of  the  string,  a  whole  series  of 
different  simple  vibrational  forms  will  generally  be  at  the  same  time  generated  in 
the  string.  For  when  the  prime  of  the  musical  tone  corresponds  to  the  prime  of 
the  string  aU  the  harmonic  upper  partials  of  the  first  correspond  to  those  of  the  ^ 
second,  and  are  hence  capable  of  exciting  the  corresponding  vibrational  forms  in 
the  string.  Generally  the  string  will  be  brought  into  as  many  forms  of  sympa- 
thetic vibration  by  the  motion  of  the  air,  as  the  analysis  of  that  motion  shews  that 
it  possesses  simple  vibrational  forms,  having  a  periodic  time  equal  to  that  of  some 
vibrational  form,  that  the  string  is  capable  of  assuming.  But  as  a  general  rule 
when  there  is  one  such  simple  vibrational  form  in  the  air,  there  are  several  such, 
and  it  will  often  be  difficult  to  determine  by  which  one,  out  of  the  many  possible 
simple  tones  which  would  produce  the  effect,  the  string  has  been  excited.  Conse- 
quently the  usual  unweighted  strings  are  not  so  convenient  for  the  determination 
of  the  pitch  of  any  simple  tones  which  exist  in  a  composite  mass  of  air,  as  the 
membranes  or  the  inclosed  air  of  resonators. 

To  make  experiments  with  the  pianoforte  on  the  sympathetic  vibrations  of 
strings,  select  a  fiat  instrument,  raise  its  lid  so  as  to  expose  the  strings,  then  press 
down  the  key  of  the  string  (for  d  suppose)  which  you  wish  to  put  into  sympathetic  m 
vibration,  but  so  slowly  that  the  hammer  does  not  strike,  and  place  a  little  chip  of 
wood  across  this  d  string.  You  will  find  the  chip  put  in  motion,  or  even  throvm 
o£f,  when  certain  other  strings  are  struck.  The  motion  of  the  chip  is  greatest  when 
one  of  the  wnder  tcmts  of  d  (p.  44^^)  is  struck,  as  c,  i^,  0,  A)^,  F,,  D,,  or  0^.  Some, 
but  much  less,  motion  also  occiurs  when  one  of  the  upper  partial  tones  of  d  is 
struck,  as  d\  g",  or  d",  but  in  this  last  case  the  chip  will  not  move  if  it  has  been 
placed  over  one  of  the  corresponding  nodes  of  the  string.  Thus  if  it  is  laid  across 
the  middle  of  the  string  it  will  be  still  for  d'  and  d'\  but  will  move  iotg".  Placed 
at  one  third  the  length  of  the  string  from  its  extremity,  it  will  not  stir  for  g",  but 
will  move  for  d'  or  d".  Finally  the  string  d  will  also  be  put  in  motion  when  an 
under  tone  of  one  of  its  upper  partial  tones  is  struck ;  for  example,  the  note/,  of  which 
the  third  partial  tone  d'  is  identical  with  the  second  partial  tone  of  d.  In  this  case 
also  the  chip  remains  at  rest  when  put  on  to  the  middle  of  the  string  c',  which  is 
its  node  for  c".  In  the  same  way  the  string  d  wiQ  move,  with  the  formation  of  -r 
two  nodes,  for  g',  g,  or  S^,  all  which  notes  have  g"  as  an  upper  partial  tone,  which 
is  also  the  third  partial  of  c'.* 

Observe  that  on  the  pianoforte,  where  one  end  of  the  strings  is  commonly 
concealed,  the  position  of  the  nodes  is  easily  found  by  pressing  the  string  gently 
on  both  sides  and  striking  the  key.  If  the  finger  is  at  a  node  the  corresponding 
upper  partial  tone  will  be  heard  purely  and  distinctly,  otherwise  the  tone  of  the 
string  is  dull  and  bad. 

As  long  as  only  one  upper  partial  tone  of  the  string  d  is  excited,  the  corre- 
sponding nodes  can  be  discovered,  and  hence  the  particular  form  of  its  vibration 
determined.    But  this  is  no  longer  possible  by  the  above  mechanical  method  when 

*  [These  experiments  oan  of  coarse  not  be  struck  and  damped.    And  this  sounding  of  c', 

conducted  on  the  usual  upright  cottage  piano.  although  unstruck,  is  itself  a  very  interesting 

Bat  the  experimenter  can  at  least  hear  the  phenomenon.    But  of  course,  as  it  depends  on 

tone  of  c\  if  c,  F,  C,  <&c.,  are  struck  and  the  ear,  it  does  not  establish  the  results  of  the 

immediately    damped,  or  if    c",  g",  c'"    are  text. — TraTislator.] 


Digitized  by 


Google 


rH^ 


^ 


48  OBJECTIVE   EXISTENCE   OF  PAKTIALS.  part  r. 

two  upper  partial  tones  are  excited,  such  as  c"  and  g",  as  would  be  the  case  if  both 
these  notes  were  struck  at  once  on  the  pianoforte,  because  the  whole  string  of  d 
would  then  be  in  motion. 

Although  the  relations  for  strings  appear  more  complicated  to  the  eye,  their 
sympathetic  vibration  is  subject  to  the  same  law  as  that  which  holds  for  resonators, 
membranes,  and  other  elastic  bodies.  The  sympathetic  vibration  is  always  deter- 
mined by  the  analysis  of  whatever  sonorous  motions  exist,  into  simple  pendular 
vibrations.  If  the  periodic  time  of  one  of  these  simple  vibrations  corresponds  to 
the  periodic  time  of  one  of  the  proper  tones  of  the  elastic  body,  that  body,  whether 
it  be  a  string,  a  membrane,  or  a  mass  of  air,  will  be  put  into  strong  sympathetic 
vibration. 

These  facts  give  a  real  objective  value  to  the  analysis  of  sonorous  motion  into 
simple  pendular  vibration,  and  no  such  value  would  attach  to  any  other  analysis. 

^  Every  individual  single  system  of  waves  formed  by  pendular  vibrations  exists  as 
an  independent  mechanical  imit,  expands,  and  sets  in  motion  other  elastic  bodies 
having  the  corresponding  proper  tone,  perfectly  undisturbed  by  any  other  simple 
tones  of  other  pitches  which  may  be  expanding  at  the  same  time,  and  which  may 
^•oceed  either  from  the  same  or  any  other  source  of  sound.  Each  single  simple 
^Tone,  then,  can,  as  we  have  seen,  be  separated  from  the  composite  mass  of  tones, 
by  mechanical  means,  namely  by  bodies,  which  will  vibrate  sympathetically  with 
it.  Hence  every  individual  partial  tone  exists  in  the  compoxmd  musical  tone 
produced  by  a  single  musical  instrument,  just  as  truly,  and  in  the  same  sense,  as  the 
different  colours  of  the  rainbow  exist  in  the  white  light  proceeding  from  the  sun 
or  any  other  luminous  body.  Light  is  also  only  a  vibrational  motion  of  a  peculiar 
elastic  medium,  the  luminous  ether,  just  as  sound  is  a  vibrational  motion  of  the 
air.  In  a  beam  of  white  Ught  there  is  a  species  of  motion  which  may  be  repre- 
sented as  the  sum  of  many  oscillatory  motions  of  various  periodic  times,  each  of 

^  which  corresponds  to  one  particular  colour  of  the  solar  spectrum.  But  of  course 
each  particle  of  ether  at  any  particular  moment  has  only  one  determinate  velocity, 
and  only  one  determinate  departure  from  its  mean  position,  just  hke  each  particle 
of  air  in  a  space  traversed  by  many  systems  of  sonorous  waves.  The  really  exist- 
ing mction  of  any  particle  of  ether  is  of  course  only  one  and  individual ;  and  our 
theoretical  treatment  of  it  as  compound,  is  in  a  certain  sense  arbitrary.  But  the 
undulatory  motion  of  hght  can  also  be  analysed  into  the  waves  corresponding  to 
the  separate  colours,  by  external  mechanical  means,  such  as  by  refraction  in  a 
prism,  or  by  transmission  through  fine  gratings,  and  each  individual  simple  wave 
of  light  corresponding  to  a  simple  colour,  exists  mechanically  by  itself,  indepen- 
dently of  any  other  colour. 

We  must  therefore  not  hold  it  to  be  an  illusion  of  the  ear,  or  to  be  mere 
imagination,  when  in  the  musical  tone  of  a  single  note  emanating  from  a  musical 
instrument,  we  distinguish  many  partial  tones,  as  I  have  found  musicians  inclined 

m  to  think,  even  when  they  have  heard  those  partial  tones  quite  distinctly  with  their 
own  ears.  If  we  admitted  this,  we  should  have  also  to  look  upon  the  colours  of 
the  spectrum  which  are  separated  from  white  light,  as  a  mere  illusion  of  the  eye. 
The  real  outward  existence  of  partial  tones  in  nature  can  be  estabHshed  at  any 
moment  by  a  sympathetically  vibrating  membrane  which  casts  up  the  sand  strewn 
upon  it. 

Finally  I  would  observe  that,  as  respects  the  conditions  of  sympathetic  vibra- 
tion, I  have  been  obliged  to  refer  frequently  to  the  mechanical  theory  of  tlie 
motion  of  air.  Since  in  the  theory  of  sound  we  have  to  deal  with  well-known 
mechanical  forces,  as  the  pressure  of  the  air,  and  with  motions  of  material 
particles,  and  not  with  any  hypothetical  explanation,  theoretical  mechanics  have 
an  unassailable  authority  in  this  department  of  science.  Of  course  those  readers 
who  are  unacquainted  with  mathematics,  must  accept  the  results  on  faith.  An 
experimental  way  of  examining  the  problems  in  question  will  be  described  in  the 
next  chapter,  in  which  the  laws  of  the  analysis  of  musical  tones  by  the  ear  have 

Digitized  by  V^OOQIC 


CHAPS,  in.  IV.      METHODS  OP  OBSERVING  PARTIAL  TONES.  49 

to  be  established.  The  experimental  proof  there  given  for  the  ear,  can  also  be 
carried  oat  in  precisely  the  same  way  for  membranes  and  masses  of  air  which 
vibrate  sympathetically,  and  the  identity  of  the  laws  in  both  cases  will  result  from 
those  investigations.* 


CHAPTER  IV. 

ON   THE   ANALYSIS  OF  MUSICAL  TONES  BY  THE   EAR. 

It  was  frequently  mentioned  in  the  preceding  chapter  that  musical  tones  could  be 
resolved  by  the  ear  alone,  unassisted  by  any  peculiar  apparatus,  into  a  series  of 
partial  tones  corresponding  to  the  simple  pendular  vibrations  in  a  mass  of  air,  that  ^ 
is,  into  the  same  constituents  as  those  into  which  the  motion  of  the  air  is  resolved 
by  the  sympathetic  vibration  of  elastic  bodies.  We  proceed  to  shew  the  correctness 
of  this  assertion. 

Any  one  who  endeavours  for  the  first  time  to  distinguish  the  upper  partial 
tones  of  a  musical  tone,  generally  finds  considerable  difficulty  in  merely  hearing 
them. 

The  analysis  of  our  sensations  when  it  cannot  be  attached  to  corresponding 
differences  in  external  objects,  meets  with  peculiar  difficulties,  the  nature  and 
significance  of  which  will  have  to  be  considered  hereafter.  The  attention  of  the 
observer  has  generally  to  be  drawn  to  the  phenomenon  he  has  to  observe,  by 
peculiar  aids  properly  selected,  until  he  knows  precisely  what  to  look  for  ;  after  he 
Las  once  succeeded,  he  wiU  be  able  to  throw  aside  such  crutches.  Similar  diffi- 
culties meet  us  in  the  observation  of  the  upper  partials  of  a  musical  tone.  I  shall 
first  give  a  description  of  such  processes  as  will  most  easily  put  an  untrained  f 
observer  into  a  position  to  recognise  upper  partial  tones,  and  I  will  remark  in 
passing  that  a  musically  trained  ear  will  not  necessarily  hear  upper  partial  tones 
with  greater  ease  and  certainty  than  an  untrained  ear.  Success  depends  rather 
upon  a  peculiar  power  of  mental  abstraction  or  a  peculiar  mastery  over  attention, 
than  upon  musical  training.  But  a  musically  trained  observer  has  an  essential 
advantage  over  one  not  so  trained  in  his  power  of  figuring  to  himself  how  the 
simple  tones  sought  for,  ought  to  sound,  whereas  the  untrained  observer  has  con- 
tmnally  to  hear  these  tones  sounded  by  other  means  in  order  to  keep  their  effect 
fresh  in  his  mind. 

First  we  must  note,  that  the  unevenly  numbered  partials,  as  the  Fifths,  Thirds, 
Sevenths,  &c.,  of  the  prime  tones,  are  usually  easier  to  hear  than  the  even  ones, 
which  are  Octaves  either  of  the  prime  tone  or  of  some  of  the  upper  partials  which 
lie  near  it,  just  as  in  a  chord  we  more  readily  diFtinguish  whether  it  contains 
Fifths  and  Thirds  than  whether  it  has  Octaves.  The  second,  fourth,  and  eighth  H 
partials  are  higher  Octaves  of  the  prime,  the  sixth  partial  an  Octave  above  the 
third  partial,  that  is,  the  Twelfth  of  the  prime  ;  and  some  practice  is  required  for 
distinguishing  these.  Among  the  uneven  partials  which  are  more  easily  dis- 
tinguished, the  first  place  must  be  assigned,  from  its  usual  loudness,  to  the  third 
partial,  the  Twelfth  of  the  prime,  or  the  Fifth  of  its  first  higher  Octave.  Then 
follows  the  fifth  partial  as  the  major  Third  of  the  prime,  and,  generally  very  faint, 
the  seventli  partial  as  the  minor  Seventhf  of  the  second  higher  Octave  of  the 
prime,  as  will  be  seen  by  their  following  expression  in  musical  notation,  for  the 
compound  tone  c. 

•  Optical  means  for  rendering  visible  weak  f  i^^  Taore  correctly  s?i6-minor  Seventh ; 

sympathetic  motions  of  sonorous  masses  of  as  the  real  minor  Seventh,  formed  by  taking 

air,  are  described  in  App.  II.    These  means  two  Fifths  down  and  then  two  Octaves  ap,  is 

are  valuable  for  demonstrating  the  facts  to  sharper  by  27  cents,  or  in  the  ratio  of  63  :  64. 

hearers  unaccustomed  to  the  observing  and  — Translator.'] 
distinguishing  musical  tones. 


Digitized  by 


GSogle 


so  METHODS  OF  OBSERVING  PARTIAL  TONES.  part  i. 


^^ 


^^^^ 


:?2Z 


-h- 


I2«.>'345  6  78 

c  &  g'  d'         e"  g"        ''h"\}       c'" 

[Cents,    o  1 200  1902  2400        2786        3102        3369        3600]  ♦ 

In  commencing  to  observe  upper  partial  tones,  it  is  advisable  just  before  pro- 
ducing the  musical  tone  itself  which  you  wish  to  analyse,  to  sound  the  note  you 
wish  to  distinguish  in  it,  very  gently,  and  if  possible  in  the  same  quality  of  tone 
as  the  compound  itself.  The  pianoforte  and  harmonium  are  well  adapted  for 
these  experiments,  because  they  both  have  upper  partial  tones  of  considerable 
power. 

II  First  gently  strike  on  a  piano  the  note  g\  as  marked  above,  and  after  letting 
the  digital  t  rise  so  as  to  damp  the  string,  strike  the  note  c,  of  which  g'  is  the 
third  partial,  with  great  force,  and  keep  your  attention  directed  to  the  pitch  of  the 
f  which  you  had  just  heard,  and  you  will  hear  it  again  in  the  compound  tone  of 
c.  Similarly,  first  stroke  the  fifth  partial  e"  gently,  and  then  c  strongly.  These 
upper  partial  tones  are  often  more  distinct  as  the  sound  dies  away,  because  they 
appear  to  lose  force  more  slowly  than  the  prime.  The  seventh  and  ninth  partialB 
h"\^  and  d^"  are  mostly  weak,  or  quite  absent  on  modem  pianos.  If  the  same  ex- 
periments are  tried  with  an  harmonium  in  one  of  its  louder  stops,  the  seventh 
partial  will  generally  be  well  heard,  and  sometimes  even  the  ninth. 

To  the  objection  which  is  sometimes  made  that  the  observer  only  imagines  he 
hears  the  partial  tone  in  the  compound,  because  he  had  just  heard  it  by  itself,  I 
need  only  remark  at  present  that  if  e!'  is  first  heard  as  a  partial  tone  of  c  on  a 
good  piano,  tuned  in  equal  temperament,  and  then  e"  is  struck  on  the  instrument 

IF  itself,  it  is  quite  easy  to  perceive  that  the  latter  is  a  httle  sharper.  This  follows 
from  the  method  of  tuning.  But  if  there  is  a  difference  in  pitch  between  the  two 
tones,  one  is  certainly  not  a  continuation  of  the  mental  effect  produced  by  the 
other.  Other  facts  wliich  completely  refute  the  above  conception,  will  be  subse- 
quently adduced. 

A  still  more  suitable  process  than  that  just  described  for  the  piano,  «an  be 
adopted  on  any  stringed  instrument,  as  the  piano,  monochord,  or  violin.  It  con- 
sists in  first  producing  the  tone  we  wish  to  hear,  as  an  harmonic,  [p.  2$d,  note]  by 
touching  the  corresponding  node  of  the  string  when  it  is  struck  or  rubbed.  The 
resemblance  of  the  tone  first  heard  to  the  corresponding  partial  of  the  compound 
is  then  much  greater,  and  the  ear  discovers  it  more  readily.  It  is  usual  to  place  a 
di\aded  scale  by  the  string  of  a  monochord,  to  facilitate  the  discovery  of  the  nodes. 
Those  for  the  third  partial,  as  shewn  in  Chap.  III.  (p.  45^),  divide  the  string  into 
three  equal  parts,  those  for  the  fifth  into  five,  and  so  on.     On  the  piano  and  violin 

II  the  position  of  these  points  is  easily  found  experimentally,  by  touching  the  string 
gently  with  the  finger  in  the  neighbourhood  of  the  node,  which  has  been  approxi- 
matively  determined  by  the  eye,  then  striking  or  bowing  the  string,  and  moving 
the  finger  about  till  the  required  harmonic  comes  out  strongly  and  purely.  By 
then  sounding  the  string,  at  one  time  with  the  finger  on  the  node,  and  at  another 
without,  we  obtain  the  required  upper  partial  at  one  time  as  an  harmonic,  and  at 
another  in  the  compound  tone  of  the  whole  string,  and  thus  learn  to  recognise  the 

f  existence  of  the  first  as  part  of  the  second,  with  comparative  ease.     Using  thin 

'    strings  which  have  loud  upper  partials,  I  have  thus  been  able  to  recognise  the 

*  [The  cents,  (see  p.  4ie2,  note)  reckoned  piano  or  org&n,  are  best  called  digitals  or 
from  the  lowest  note,  are  assigned  on  the  finger-keys,  on  the  analogy  of  pedals  and  foot- 
supposition  that  the  harmonics  are  perfect,  keys  on  the  organ.  The  word  key  having 
as  on  the  Harmonica!,  not  tempered  as  on  another  musical  sense,  namely,  the  scale  *n 
the  pianoforte.  See  also  diagram,  p.  22c —  which  a  piece  of  music  is  written,  will  without 
Translator.}  prefix  be  confined  to  this  meaning. — Tran^- 

t  [The  keys  played  by  the  fingers  on  a  lator.] 

Digitized  by  VjOOQlC 


CHAP.  IV.        METHODS  OP  OBSEBVING  PARTIAL  TONES.  51 

partials  separately,  up  to  the  sixteenth.    Those  which  lie  still  higher  are  too  near 
to  each  other  in  pitch  for  the  ear  to  separate  them  readily. 

In  such  experiments  I  recommend  the  following  process.  Touch  the  node  of 
the  string  on  the  pianoforte  or  monochord  with  a  camel* s-hair  pencil,  strike  the 
note,  and  immediately  remove  the  pencil  from  the  string.  If  the  pencil  has  been 
pressed  tightly  on  the  string,  we  either  continue  to  hear  the  required  partial  as  an 
harmonic,  or  else  in  addition  hear  the  prime  tone  gently  sounding  with  it.  On 
repeating  the  excitement  of  the  string,  and  continuing  to  press  more  and  more 
lightly  with  the  camel's-hair  pencil,  and  at  last  removing  the  pencil  entirely,  the 
prime  tone  of  the  string  wiU  be  heard  more  and  more  distinctly  with  the  harmonic 
till  we  have  finally  the  full  natural  musical  tone  of  the  string.  By  this  means 
we  obtain  a  series  of  gradual  transitional  stages  between  the  isolated  partial  and 
the  compound  tone,  in  which  the  first  is  readily  retained  by  the  ear.  By  applying 
this  last  process  I  have  generally  succeeded  in  making  perfectly  untrained  ears  f 
recognise  the  existence  of  upper  partial  tones. 

It  is  at  first  more  difficult  to  hear  the  upper  partials  on  most  wind  instruments 
and  in  the  human  voice,  than  on  stringed  instruments,  harmoniums,  and  the  more 
penetrating  stops  of  an  organ,  because  it  is  then  not  so  easy  first  to  produce  the 
upper  partial  softly  in  the  same  quality  of  tone.  But  still  a  little  practice  suffices 
to  lead  the  ear  to  the  required  partial  tone,  by  previously  touching  it  on  the  piano. 
The  partial  tones  of  the  himian  voice  are  comparatively  most  difficult  to  distinguish 
for  reasons  which  will  be  given  subsequently.  Nevertheless  they  were  distin- 
gnished  even  by  Eameau*  without  the  assistance  of  any  apparatus.  The  process 
is  as  follows : — 

Get  a  powerful  bass  voice  to  sing  ^  to  the  vowel  0,  in  sore  [more  like  aw 
in  SOU)  than  o  in  so],  gently  touch  h'\}  on  the  piano,  which  is  the  Twelfth,  or 
third  partial  tone  of  the  note  e|;>,  and  let  its  sound  die  away  while  you  are  listening 
to  it  attentively.  The  note  h'\^  on  the  piano  will  appear  really  not  to  die  away,  % 
but  to  keep  on  sounding,  even  when  its  string  is  damped  by  removing  the  finger 
from  the  digital,  because  the  ear  unconsciously  passes  from  the  tone  of  the  piano 
to  the  partial  tone  of  the  same  pitch  produced  by  the  singer,  and  takes  the  latter 
for  a  continuation  of  the  former.  But  when  the  finger  is  removed  from  the  key, 
and  the  damper  has  fallen,  it  is  of  course  impossible  that  the  tone  of  the  string 
should  have  continued  soimding.  To  make  the  experiment  for  g"  the  fifth  partial, 
or  major  Third  of  the  second  Octave  above  «(;>,  the  voice  should  sing  to  the  vowel 
kmfatlier. 

The  resonators  described  in  the  last  chapter  furnish  an  excellent  means  for 
this  purpose,  and  can  be  used  for  the  tones  of  any  musical  instrument.  On  apply, 
ing  to  the  ear  the  resonator  corresponding  to  any  given  upper  partial  of  the  com- 
pound c,  such  as  g',  this  g'  is  rendered  much  more  powerful  when  c  is  sounded. 
Now  hearing  and  distinguishing  g'  in  this  case  by  no  means  proves  that  the  ear 
alone  and  without  this  apparatus  would  hear  g'  as  part  of  the  compound  c.  But  % 
the  increase  of  the  loudness  of  g'  caused  by  the  resonator  may  be  used  to  direct 
the  attention  of  the  ear  to  the  tone  it  is  required  to  distinguish.  On  gradually 
removing  the  resonator  from  the  ear,  the  force  of  g'  will  decrease.  But  the 
attention  once  directed  to  it  by  this  means,  remains  more  readily  fixed  upon 
it,  and  the  observer  continues  to  hear  this  tone  in  the  natural  and  unchanged 
compound  tone  of  the  given  note,  even  with  his  unassisted  ear.  The  sole  office 
of  the  resonators  in  this  case  is  to  direct  the  attention  of  the  ear  to  the  required 
tone. 

By  frequently  instituting  similar  experiments  for  perceiving  the  upper  partial 
tones,  the  observer  comes  to  discover  them  more  and  more  easily,  till  he  is  finally 
able  to  dispense  with  any  aids.  But  a  certain  amount  of  undisturbed  concentration 
is  always  necessary  for  analysing  musical  tones  by  the  ear  alone,  and  hence  the 
use  of  resonators  is  quite  indispensable  for  an  accurate  comparison  of  different 


*  Nouveau  Syatmne  de  Musique  thiorique.    Paris :  1726.    Preface. 

Digitized  by 


<^i:iPgle 


52  PEOOF  OF  OHM'S  LAW.  pairt  i. 

qualities  of  tones,  especially  in  respect  to  the  weaker  upper  partials.  At  least,  I 
must  confess,  that  my  own  attempts  to  discover  the  upper  partial  tones  in  the 
human  voice,  and  to  determine  their  differences  for  different  vowels,  were  most 
unsatisfactory  until  I  applied  the  resonators. 

We  now  proceed  to  prove  that  the  human  ear  really  does  analyse  musical 
tones  according  to  the  law  of  simple  vibrations.  Since  it  is  not  possible  to  insti- 
tute an  exact  comparison  of  the  strength  of  our  sensations  for  different  simple 
tones,  we  must  confine  ourselves  to  proving  that  when  an  analysis  of  a  composite 
tone  into  simple  vibrations,  effected  by  theoretic  calculation  or  by  sympathetic 
resonance,  shews  that  certain  upper  partial  tones  are  absent,  the  ear  also  does 
not  perceive  them. 

The  tones  of  strings  are  again  best  adapted  for  conducting  this  proof,  because 
they  admit  of  many  alterations  in  their  quality  of  tone,  according  to  the  manner 
II  and  the  spot  in  which  they  are  excited,  and  also  because  the  theoretic  or  experi- 
mental analysis  is  most  easily  and  completely  performed  for  this  case.  Thomas 
Young*  first  shewed  that  when  a  string  is  plucked  or  struck,  or,  as  we  may  add, 
bowed  at  any  point  in  its  length  which  is  the  node  of  any  of  its  so-called 
liarmonics,  those  simple  vibrational  forms  of  the  string  which  have  a  node  in  that 
point  are  not  contained  in  the  compound  vibrational  form.  Hence,  if  we  attack 
the  string  at  its  middle  point,  all  the  simple  vibrations  due  to  the  evenly  numbered 
partials,  each  of  which  has  a  note  at  that  point,  will  be  absent.  This  gives  the 
sound  of  the  string  a  peculiarly  hollow  or  nasal  twang.  If  we  excite  the  string  at 
^  of  its  length,  the  vibrations  corresponding  to  the  third,  sixth,  and  ninth  partials 
will  be  absent ;  if  at  ^,  then  those  corresponding  to  the  fourth,  eighth,  and  twelfth 
partials  will  fail ;  and  so  on.f 

This  result  of  mathematical  theory  is  confirmed,  in  the  first  place,  by  analys- 
ing the  compound  tone  of  the  string  by  sympathetic  resonance,  either  by  the 
f  resonators  or  by  other  strings.  The  experiments  may  be  easily  made  on  the 
pianoforte.  Press  down  the  digitals  for  the  notes  c  and  c',  without  allowing  the 
hammer  to  strike,  so  as  merely  to  free  them  from  their  dampers,  and  then  pluck 
the  string  c  with  the  nail  till  it  sounds.  On  damping  the  c  string  the  higher  & 
will  echo  the  sound,  except  in  the  particular  case  when  the  c  string  has  been 
plucked  exactly  at  its  middle  point,  which  is  the  point  where  it  would  have  to  be 
touched  in  order  to  give  its  first  harmonic  when  struck  by  the  hammer. 

If  we  touch  the  c  string  at  i  or  f  its  length,  and  strike  it  with  the  hammer, 
we  obtain  the  harmonic  g* ;  and  if  the  damper  of  the  g^  is  raised,  this  string  echoes 
the  sound.  But  if  we  pluck  the  c  string  with  the  nail,  at  either  1  or  §  its  length, 
g'  is  not  echoed,  as  it  will  be  if  the  c  string  is  plucked  at  any  other  spot. 

In  the  same  way  observations  with  the  resonators  shew  that  when  the  c  string 

is  plucked  at  its  middle  the  Octave  c'  is  missing,  and  when  at  ^  or  ^  its  length  the 

Twelfth  g'  is  absent.     The  analysis  of  the  sound  of  a  string  by  the  sympathetic 

%  resonance  of  strings  or  resonators,  consequently  fully  confirms  Thomas  Young's 

law. 

But  for  the  vibration  of  strings  we  have  a  more  direct  means  of  analysis  than 
that  furnished  by  sympathetic  resonance.  If  we,  namely,  touch  a  vibrating  string 
gently  for  a  moment  with  the  finger  or  a  camePs-hair  pencil,  we  damp  all  those 
simple  vibrations  which  have  no  node  at  the  point  touched.  Those  vibrations, 
however,  which  have  a  node  there  are  not  damped,  and  hence  will  continue  to 
sound  without  the  others.  Consequently,  if  a  string  has  been  made  to  speak  in 
any  way  whatever,  and  we  wish  to  know  whether  there  exists  among  its  simple 
vibrations  one  corresponding  to  the  Twelfth  of  the  prime  tone,  we  need  only  touch 
one  of  the  nodes  of  this  vibrational  form  at  ^  or  f  the  length  of  the  string,  in 
order  to  reduce  to  silence  all  simple  tones  which  have  no  such  node,  and  leave  the 
Twelfth  sounding,  if  it  were  there.    If  neither  it,  nor  any  of  the  sixth,  ninth, 

*  London.    Philosophical  Transactio7iSt  1800,  vol.  i.  p.  137. 
t  See  Appendix  III. 

Digitized  by  VjOOQIC 


CHAP.  IV.  PEOOF  OF  OHM'S  LAW.  53 

twelfth,  &c.,  of  the  partial  tones  were  present,  giving  corresponding  harmonics, 
the  string  will  be  reduced  to  absolute  silence  by  this  contact  of  the  finger. 

Press  down  one  of  the  digitals  of  a  piano,  in  order  to  free  a  string  from  its 
damper.  Pluck  the  string  at  its  middle  point,  and  immediately  touch  it  there. 
The  string  will  be  completely  silenced,  shewing  that  plucking  it  in  its  middle 
excited  none  of  the  evenly  numbered  partials  of  its  compound  tone.  Pluck  it  at  ^  or  ^ 
its  length,  and  immediately  touch  it  in  the  same  place ;  the  string  will  be  silent, 
proving  the  absence  of  the  third  partial  tone.  Pluck  the  string  anywhere  else 
than  in  the  points  named,  and  the  second  partial  will  be  heard  when  the  middle  is 
touched,  the  third  when  the  string  is  touched  at  ^  or  §  of  its  length. 

The  agreement  of  this  kind  of  proof  with  the  results  from  sympathetic  reso- 
nance, is  well  adapted  for  the  experimental  establishment  of  the  proposition  based 
in  the  last  chapter  solely  upon  the  results  of  mathematical  theory,  namely,  that 
sympathetic  vibration  occurs  or  not,  according  as  the  corresponding  simple  H 
vibrations  are  or  are  not  contained  in  the  compound  motion.  In  the  last  described 
method  of  analysing  the  tone  of  a  string,  we  are  quite  independent  of  the  theory 
of  sympathetic  vibration,  and  the  simple  vibrations  of  strings  are  exactly  charac- 
terised and  recognisable  by  their  nodes.  If  the  compound  tones  admitted  of  being 
analysed  by  sympathetic  resonance  according  to  any  other  vibrational  forms  except 
those  of  simple  vibration,  this  agreement  could  not  exist. 

If,  after  having  thus  experimentally  proved  the  correctness  of  Thomas  Young's 
law,  we  try  to  analyse  the  tones  of  strings  by  the  unassisted  ear,  we  shall  continue 
to  find  complete  agreement.*  If  we  pluck  or  strike  a  string  in  one  of  its  nodes, 
ail  those  upper  partial  tones  of  the  compound  tone  of  the  string  to  which  the  node 
belongs,  disappear  for  the  ear  also,  but  they  are  heard  if  the  string  is  plucked  at 
any  other  place.  Thus,  if  the  string  c  be  plucked  at  ^  its  length,  the  partial  tone 
g*  cannot  be  heard,  but  if  the  string  be  plucked  at  only  a  little  distance  from  this 
point  the  partial  tone  g^  is  distinctly  audible.  Hence  the  ear  analyses  the  sound  f 
of  a  string  into  precisely  the  same  constituents  as  are  found  by  sympathetic  reso- 
nance, that  is,  into  simple  tones,  according  to  Ohm*s  definition  of  this  conception. 
These  experiments  are  also  well  adapted  to  shew  that  it  is  no  mere  play  of  imagina- 
tion when  we  hear  upper  partial  tones,  as  some  people  believe  on  hearing  them  for 
the  first  time,  for  those  tones  are  not  heard  when  they  do  not  exist. 

The  following  modification  of  this  process  is  also  very  well  adapted  to  make 
the  upper  partial  tones  of  strings  audible.  First,  strike  alternately  in  rhythmical 
sequence,  the  third  and  fourth  partial  tone  of  the  string  alone,  by  damping  it  in  the 
corresponding  nodes,  and  request  the  listener  to  observe  the  simple  melody  thus 
produced.  Then  strike  the  undamped  string  alternately  and  in  the  same  rhythmical 
sequence,  in  these  nodes,  and  thus  reproduce  the  same  melody  in  the  upper  partials, 
which  the  listener  will  then  easily  recognise.  Of  course,  in  order  to  hear  the 
third  partial,  we  must  strike  the  string  in  the  node  of  the  fourth,  and  conversely. 

The  compound  tone  of  a  plucked  string  is  also  a  remarkably  striking  example  % 
of  the  power  of  the  ear  to  analyse  into  a  long  series  of  partial  tones,  a  motion 
which  the  eye  and  the  imagination  are  able  to  conceive  in  a  much  simpler  manner. 
A  string,  which  is  pulled  aside  by  a  sharp  point,  or  the  finger  nail,  assumes  the 
form  fig.  18,  A  (p.  S4a),  before  it  is  released.  It  then  passes  through  the  series  of 
forms,  fig.  18,  B,  C,  D,  E,  F,  till  it  reaches  G,  which  is  the  inversion  of  A,  and 
then  returns,  through  the  same,  to  A  again.  Hence  it  alternates  between  the  forms 
A  and  G.  AU  these  forms,  as  is  clear,  are  composed  of  three  straight  lines,  and 
on  expressing  the  velocity  of  the  individual  points  of  the  strings  by  vibrational 
curves,  these  would  have  the  same  form.  Now  the  string  scarcely  imparts  any 
perceptible  portion  of  its  own  motion  directly  to  the  air.  Scarcely  any  audible 
tone  results  when  both  ends  of  a  string  are  fastened  to  immovable  supports,  as 
metal  bridges,  which  are  again  fastened  to  the  walls  of  a  room.    The  sound  of 

*  See  Brandt  in  Poggcndorff's  Annalen  der  Physik,  vol.  cxii.  p.  324,  where  this  fact  ia 
proved. 


Digitized  by  VjOOQ IC 


54 


PROOF  OF  OHM'S  LAW. 


PART  I. 


Fio.  x8. 


the  string  reaches  the  air  through  that  one  of  its  extremities  which  rests  upon 
a  bridge  standing  on  an  elastic  sounding  board.  Hence  the  sound  of  the  string 
essentially  depends  on  the  motion  of  this 
extremity,  through  the  pressure  which  it 
exerts  on  the  sounding  board.  The  magni- 
tude of  this  pressure,  as  it  alters  periodically 
with  the  time,  is  shewn  in  fig.  19,  where 
the  height  of  the  line  h  h  corresponds  to 
the  amount  of  pressure  exerted  on  the  bridge 
by  that  extremity  of  the  string  when  the 
string  is  at  rest.  Along  h  h  suppose 
lengths  to  be  set  off  corresponding  to  con- 
secutive   intervals    of   time,    the    vertical 

%  heights  of  the  broken  line  above  or  below 
h  h  represent  the  corresponding  augmenta- 
tions or  diminutions  of  pressure  at  those 
times.  The  pressure  of  the  string  on  the 
sounding  board  consequently  alternates,  as 
the  figure  shews,  between  a  higher  and  a 
lower  value.  For  some  time  the  greater 
pressure  remains  unaltered ;  then  the  lower 
suddenly  ensues,  and  likewise  remains  for  a 
time  unaltered.  The  letters  a  to  g  in  fig.  19 
correspond  to  the  times  at  which  the  string 
assumes  the  forms  A  to  G  in  fig.  18.  It  is  this  alteration  between  a  greater  and 
a  smaller  pressure  which  produces  the  sound  in  the  air.  We  cannot  but  feel 
astonished  that  a  motion  produced  by  means  so  simple  and  so  easy  to  comprehend, 

f  should  be  analysed  by  the  ear  into  such  a  complicated  sum  of  simple  tones.  For 
the  eye  and  the  understanding  the  action  of  the  string  on  the  sounding  board  can 
be  figured  with  extreme  simplicity.  What  has  the  simple  broken  line  of  fig.  19 
to  do  with  wave*curv6S,  which,  in    the  course  of  one  of  their  periods,   shew 


1-I--I-U: 


1)    c    d 


4...|.,..|-+.H.-4-|-4-. 


f   g  t 


Fig. 


h    a 


19. 


3,  4,  5,  up  to  16,  and  more,  crests  and  troughs  ?  This  is  one  of  the  most  striking 
examples  of  the  different  ways  in  which  eye  and  ear  comprehend  a  periodic 
motion. 

There  is  no  sonorous  body  whose  motions  under  varied  conditions  can  be  so 
^  completely  calculated  theoretically  and  contrasted  with  observation  as  a  string. 
The  following  are  examples  in  which  theory  can  be  compared  with  analysis  by 
ear: — 

I  have  discovered  a  means  of  exciting  simple  pendular  vibrations  in  the  air.  A 
tuning-fork  when  struck  gives  no  harmonic  upper  partial  tones,  or,  at  most,  traces 
of  them  when  it  is  brought  into  such  excessively  strong  vibration  that  it  no  longer 
exactly  follows  the  law  of  the  pendulum.*  On  the  other  hand,  tuning-forks  have 
some  very  high  inharmonic  secondary  tones,  which  produce  that  peculiar  sharp 


*  [On  all  ordinary  tuning-forks  between  a 
and  d"  in  pitch,  I  have  been  able  to  hear  the 
Beoond  partial  or  Octave  of  the  prime.  In 
Bome  low  forks  this  Octave  is  so  powerful  that 
on  pressing  the  handle  of  the  fork  against  the 
table^  the  prime  quite  disappears  and  the 
Octave  only  is  heard,  .and  this  has  often 
proved  a  source  of  embarrassment  in  tuning 
the  forks,  or  in  counting  beats  to  determine 


pitch  numbers.  But  the  prime  can  always  be 
heard  when  the  fork  is  held  to  the  ear  or  over 
a  properly  tuned  resonance  jar,  as  described  in 
this  paragraph.  I  tune  such  jars  by  pouring 
water  in  or  out  until  the  resonance  is  strongest, 
and  then  I  register  the  height  of  the  water 
and  pitch  of  the  fork  for  future  use  on  a  slip 
of  paper  gummed  to  the  side  of  the  jar.  I 
have  found  that  it  is  not  at  all  necessary  to 


Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


CHAP.  IV.  PBOOP  OF  OHM'S  LAW.  55 

tinkling  of  the  fork  at  the  moment  of  being  struck,  and  generally  become  rapidly 
inaudible.  If  the  tuning-fork  is  held  in  the  fingers,  it  imparts  very  little  of  its 
tone  to  the  air,  and  cannot  be  heard  unless  it  is  held  close  to  the  ear.  Instead  of 
holding  it  in  the  fingers,  we  may  screw  it  into  a  thick  board,  on  the  under  side  of 
which  some  pieces  of  india-rubber  tubing  have  been  fastened.  When  this  is  laid 
upon  a  table,  the  india-rubber  tubes  on  which  it  is  supported  convey  no  sound  to 
the  table,  and  the  tone  of  the  tuning-fork  is  so  weak  that  it  may  be  considered  in- 
audible. Now  if  the  prongs  of  the  fork  be  brought  near  a  resonance  chamber  *  of 
a  bottle-form  of  such  a  size  and  shape  that,  when  we  blow  over  its  mouth,  the  air 
it  contains  gives  a  tone  of  the  same  pitch  as  the  fork's,  the  air  within  this  chamber 
vibrates  sympathetically,  and  the  tone  of  the  fork  is  thus  conducted  with  great 
strength  to  the  outer  air.  Now  the  higher  secondary  tones  of  such  resonance 
chambers  are  also  inharmonic  to  the  prime  tone,  and  in  general  the  secondary 
tones  of  the  chambers  correspond  neither  with  the  harmonic  nor  the  inharmonic  H 
secondary  tones  of  the  forks ;  this  can  be  determined  in  each  particular  case  by 
producing  the  secondary  tones  of  the  bottle  by  stronger  blowing,  and  discovering 
those  of  the  forks  with  the  help  of  strings  set  into  sympathetic  vibration,  as  will 
be  presently  described.  If,  then,  only  one  of  the  tones  of  the  fork,  namely  the 
prime  tone,  corresponds  with  one  of  the  tones  of  the  chamber,  this  alone  will  be 
reinforced  by  sympathetic  vibration,  and  this  alone  will  be  communicated  to  the 
external  air,  and  thus  conducted  to  the  observer's  ear.  The  examination  of  the 
motion  of  the  air  by  resonators  shews  that  in  this  case,  provided  the  tuning-fork  be 
not  set  into  too  violent  motion,  no  tone  but  the  prime  is  present,  and  in  such  case 
the  unassisted  ear  hears  only  a  single  simple  tone,  namely  the  common  prime  of 
the  tuning-fork  and  of  the  chamber,  without  any  accompanying  upper  partial  tones. 

The  tone  of  a  tuning-fork  can  also  be  purified  from  secondary  tones  by  placing 
its  handle  upon  a  string  and  moving  it  so  near  to  the  bridge  that  one  of  the  proper 
tones  of  the  section  of  string  lying  between  the  fork  and  the  bridge  is  the  same  as  ^ 
that  of  the  tuning-fork.  The  string  then  begins  to  vibrate  strongly,  and  conducts 
the  tone  of  the  tuning-fork  with  great  power  to  the  sounding  board  and  surround- 
ing air,  whereas  the  tone  is  scarcely,  if  at  all,  heard  as  long  as  the  above-named 
section  is  not  in  unison  with  the  tone  of  the  fork.  In  this  way  it  is  easy  to  find 
the  lengths  of  string  which  correspond  to  the  prime  and  upper  partial  tones  of  the 
fork,  and  accurately  determine  the  pitch  of  the  latter.  If  this  experiment  is  con- 
ducted with  ordinary  strings  which  are  uniform  throughout  their  length,  we  shield 
the  ear  from  the  inharmonic  secondary  tones  of  the  fork,  but  not  firom  the  harmonic 
upper  partials,  which  are  sometimes  faintly  present  when  the  fork  is  made  to 
vibrate  strongly.  Hence  to  conduct  this  experiment  in  such  a  way  as  to  create 
purely  pendular  vibrations  of  the  air,  it  is  best  to  weight  one  point  of  the  string,  if 
only  so  much  as  by  letting  a  drop  of  melting  sealing-wax  fall  upon  it.  This  causes 
the  upper  proper  tones  of  the  string  itself  to  be  inharmonic  to  the  prime  tone,  and 
hence  there  is  a  distinct  interval  between  the  points  where  the  fork  must  be  placed  f 
to  bring  out  the  prime  tone  and  its  audible  Octave,  if  it  exists. 

In  most  other  cases  the  mathematical  analysis  of  the  motions  of  sound  is  not 
nearly  far  enough  advanced  to  determine  with  certainty  what  upper  partials  will 
be  present  and  what  intensity  they  will  possess.  In  circular  plates  and  stretched 
membranes  which  are  struck,  it  is  theoretically  possible  to  do  so,  but  their  inhar- 

ptit  the  fork  into  excessively  strong  vibration  of  Chap.  VII.,  and  Prof.  Preyer's  in  App.  XX. 

in  order  to  make  the  Octave  sensible.    Thus,  sect.  L.  art.  4,  c.     The  conditions  according 

taking  a  fork  of  232  and  another  of  468  vibra-  to  Eoenig  that  tuning-forks  should  have  no 

tions,  after  striking  them  both,  and  letting  the  upper  partials  are  given  in  App.  XX.  sect.  L. 

deeper  fork  spend  most  of  its  energy  until  I  art.  2,  a. — Tranakdcyr.] 

could  not  see  the  vibrations  with  the  eye  at  all,  *  Either  a  bottle  of  a  proper  size,  which 
the  beats  were  heard  distinctly,  when  I  pressed  can  readily  be  more  accurately  tuned  by  pour- 
both  on  to  a  table,  and  continued  to  be  heard  ing  oil  or  water  into  it,  or  a  tube  of  i>asteboard 
even  after  the  forks  themselves  were  separately  quite  closed  at  one  end,  and  having  a  small 
inaudible.  See  also  Prof.  Helmhoitz's  experi-  round  opening  at  the  other.  See  the  proper 
ments  on  a  fork  of  64  vibrations  at  the  close  sizes  of  such  resonance  chambers  in  App.  IV. 

Digitized  by  V^OOQIC 


St5 


PROOF  OF  OHM'S  LAW. 


PAST  I. 


monic  secondary  tones  are  so  numerous  and  so  nearly  of  the  same  pitch  that  most 
observers  would  probably  fail  to  separate  them  satisfactorily.  On  elastic  rods,  how- 
ever, the  secondary  tones  are  very  distant  from  each  other,  and  are  inharmonic,  so 
that  they  can  be  readily  distinguished  from  each  other  by  the  ear.  The  following 
are  the  proper  tones  of  a  rod  which  is  free  at  both  ends ;  the  vibrational  number 
of  the  prime  tone  taken  to  be  c,  is  reckoned  as  i : — 


Pitch  Number 

Cent«» 

Notation 

Prime  tone 

Second  proper  tone 

Third  proper  tone 

Fourth  proper  tone 

I -0000 

27576 
5-4041 

I3-344* 

0 

1200+556 
2400+521 
3600  +  886 

C 

f      +0-2 
f      4  0-I 
a"  -o-i 

The  notation  is  adapted  to  the  equal  temperament,  and  the  appended  fractions 
"  are  parts  of  the  interval  of  a  complete  tone. 

Where  we  are  unable  to  execute  the  theoretical  analysis  of  the  motion,  we  can, 
at  any  rate,  by  means  of  resonators  and  other  sympathetically  vibrating  bodies, 
analyse  any  individual  musical  tone  that  is  produced,  and  then  compare  this 
analysis,  which  is  determined  by  the  laws  of  sympathetic  vibration,  with  that 
effected  by  the  unassisted  ear.  The  latter  is  naturally  much  less  sensitive  than 
one  armed  with  a  resonator ;  so  that  it  is  frequently  impossible  for  the  unarmed 
ear  to  recognise  amongst  a  number  of  other  stronger  simple  tones  those  which  the 
resonator  itself  can  only  faintly  indicate.  On  the  other  hand,  so  far  as  my  ex- 
perience goes,  there  is  complete  agreement  to  this  extent :  the  ear  recognises  with- 
out resonators  the  simple  tones  which  the  resonators  greatly  reinforce,  and  perceives 
no  upper  partial  tone  which  the  resonator  does  not  indicate.  To  verify  this  con- 
clusion, I  performed  numerous  experiments,  both  with  the  human  voice  and  the 
harmonium,  and  they  all  confirmed  it.f 
If  By  the  above  experiments  the  proposition  enunciated  and  defended  by  G.  S. 
Ohm  must  be  regarded  as  proved,  viz.  that  the  human  ear  perceives  pendular  vibra- 
tions alone  as  simple  tones,  and  resolves  all  other  periodic  motions  of  the  air  into 
a  series  of  pendular  vibrations ^  hearing  the  series  of  simple  tones  which  correspond 
tvith  these  simple  vibrations. 

Calling,  then,  as  already  defined  (in  pp.  23,  24  and  note),  the  sensation  excited 
in  the  ear  by  any  periodical  motion  of  the  air  a  musical  tone,  and  the  sensation 
excited  by  a  simple  pendular  vibration  a  simple  tone,  the  rule  asserts  that  the 
sensation  of  a  musical  tone  is  compoufided  out  of  the  sensations  of  several  simple 
tones.  In  particular,  we  shall  henceforth  call  the  sound  produced  by  a  single 
sonorous  body  its  (simple  or  compound)  tone,  and  the  sound  produced  by  several 
musical  instruments  acting  at  the  same  time  a  composite  tone,  consisting  generally 
of  several  (simple  or  compound)  tones.    If,  then,  a  single  note  is  sounded  on  a 


m  *  [For  cents  see  note  p.  ^id.  As  a  Tone  is 
200  ct.,  O'l  Tone  "  20  ct.,  these  would  give  for 
the  Author's  notation  /  +  40  ct.,  /'  +  20  ct.,  a'" 
- 10  ct.,  whereas  the  column  of  cents  shews 
that  they  are  more  accurately  /  +  56  ct.,  /'  + 
21  ct.,  a'" —14  ct.  For  convenience,  the  cents 
for  Octaves  are  separated,  thus  1200+556  in 
place  of  1756,  but  this  separation  is  quite 
unnecessary.  The  cents  again  shew  the  inter- 
vals of  the  inharmonic  partial  tones  without 
any  assumption  as  to  the  value  of  the  prime. 
By  a  misprint  in  all  the  German  editions, 
followed  in  the  first  English  edition,  the  second 
proper  tone  was  made/— 0*2  in  place  of  /  + 
©•2. — Translator.] 

t  [In  my  *  Notes  of  Observations  on  Musi- 
cal Beats,'  Proceedings  of  the  Royal  Society, 
May  1880,  vol.  xxx.  p.  531,  largely  cited  in 
App.  XX.  sect.  B.  No.  7, 1  showed  Uiat  I  was 
able  to  determine  the  pitch  numbers  of  deep 


reed  tones,  by  the  beats  (Chap.  VIII.)  that  their 
upper  partials  made  with  the  primes  of  a  set  of 
Scheibler's  tuning-forks.  The  correctness  of 
the  process  was  proved  by  the  fact  that  the 
results  obtained  ftrom  different  partials  of  the 
same  reed  tone,  which  were  made  to  beat  with 
different  forks,  gave  the  same  pitch  numbers 
for  the  primes,  within  one  or  two  hundredths  of 
a  vibration  in  a  second.  I  not  only  employed 
such  low  partials  as  3,  4,  5  for  one  tone,  and 
4,  5,  6  for  others,  but  I  determined  the  pitch 
number  31*47,  by  partials  7,  8,  9,  10,  11,  12, 
13,  and  the  pitch  number  15*94  by  partials  25 
and  27.  The  objective  reality  of  these  ex- 
tremely high  upper  partials,  and  their  inde- 
pendence of  resonators  or  resonance  jars,  was 
therefore  conclusively  shewn.  On  the  Har- 
monical  the  beats  of  the  i6th  partial  of  C  66, 
with  c'",  when  slightly  flattened  by  pressirg  the 
note  lightly  down,  are  very  cleai.— Translator,  j 

Digitized  by.V^jOOQlC 


CHAP.  IV..        DIFFICULTIES  IN  OBSERVING  PAETIALS.  57 

masical  instnunent,  as  a  violin,  trmnpet,  organ,  or  by  a  singing  voice,  it  mast  be 
called  in  exact  language  a  tone  of  the  instrument  in  question.  This  is  also  the 
ordinary  language,  but  it  did  not  then  imply  that  the  tone  might  be  compound. 
When  the  tone  is,  as  usual,  a  compound  tone,  it  will  be  distinguished  by  this  term, 
or  the  abridgment,  a  compound ;  while  tone  is  a  general  term  which  includes  both 
simple  and  compound  tones.*  The  prime  tone  is  generally  louder  than  any  of  the 
upper  partial  tones,  and  hence  it  alone  generally  determines  the  pitch  of  the  com- 
pound. The  tone  produced  by  any  sonorous  body  reduces  to  a  single  simple  tone 
in  very  few  cases  indeed,  as  the  tone  of  tuning-forks  imparted  to  the  air  by  reso- 
nance chambers  in  the  manner  already  described.  The  tones  of  wide-stopped 
oigan  pipes  when  gently  blown  are  almost  free  from  upper  partials,  and  are  accom- 
panied only  by  a  rush  of  wind. 

It  is  well  known  that  this  union  of  several  simple  tones  into  one  compound 
tone,  which  is  naturally  effected  in  the  tones  produced  by  most  musical  instruments,  ^ 
is  artificially  imitated  on  the  organ  by  peculiar  mechanical  contrivances.  The 
tones  of  organ  pipes  are  comparatively  poor  in  upper  partials.  When  it  is  desirable 
to  use  a  stop  of  incisive  penetrating  quality  of  tone  and  great  power,  the  wide  pipes 
{principal  register  and  weitgedackt  t)  are  not  sufficient ;  their  tone  is  too  soft,  too 
defective  in  upper  partials  ;  and  the  narrow  pipes  (getgen-register  and  quintaten  i) 
are  also  unsuitable,  because,  although  more  incisive,  their  tone  is  weak.  For  such 
occasions,  then,  as  in  accompanying  congregational  singing,  recourse  is  had  to  the 
compound  stops. §  In  these  stops  every  key  is  connected  with  a  larger  or  smaller 
series  of  pipes,  which  it  opens  simultaneously,  and  which  give  the  prime  tone  and 
a  certain  number  of  the  lower  upper  partials  of  the  compound  tone  of  the  note  in 
question.  It  is  very  usual  to  connect  the  upper  Octave  with  the  prime  tone,  and 
after  that  the  Twelfth.  The  more  complex  compounds  (comet  §)  give  the  first  six 
partial  tones,  that  is,  in  addition  to  the  two  Octaves  of  the  prime  tone  and  its 
Twelfth,  the  higher  major  Third,  and  the  Octave  of  the  Twelfth.  This  is  as  much  ^ 
of  the  series  of  upper  partials  as  belongs  to  the  tones  of  a  major  chord.  But 
to  prevent  these  compound  stops  from  being  insupportably  noisy,  it  is  necessary 
to  reinforce  the  deeper  tones  of  each  note  by  other  rows  of  pipes,  for  in  all  natural 
tones  which  are  suited  for  musical  purposes  the  higher  partials  decrease  in  force  as 
they  rise  in  pitch.  This  has  to  be  regarded  in  their  imitation  by  compound  stops. 
These  compound  stops  were  a  monster  in  the  path  of  the  old  musical  theory,  which 
'was  acquainted  only  with  the  prime  tones  of  compounds;  but  the  practice  of 
organ-builders  and  organists  necessitated  their  retention,  and  when  they  are 
suitably  arranged  and  properly  applied,  they  form  a  very  effective  musical  apparatus. 

*  [Here,  again,  as  on  pp.  23,  24, 1  have,  in  toned  diapason,  eight  feet.*    Hopkins,  Organ, 

the  translation,  been   necessarily  obliged  to  p.  445.    *  A  manual  stop  of  eight  feet,  produ- 

deriate  slightly  from  the  original.    Klang,  as  oing  a  pungent  tone  very  like   that  of  the 

here  defined,  embraces  Ton  as  a  particular  Gamba,  except  that  the  pipes,  being  of  larger 

case.    I  use  tone  for  the  general  term,  and  scale,  speak  quicker  and  produce  a  fuller  tone. 

compound  tone  and  simple  tone  for  the  two  Examples  of  the    stop    exist  at    Doncaster,  ir 

particular  cases.    Thus,  as  presently  mentioned  the  Temple    Church,  and  in  the  Exchange 

in  the  text,  the  tone  produced  by  a  tuning-fork  Organ  at  Northampton.'    Ibid.  p.    138.    For 

held  over  a  proper  resonance  chamber  we  know,  quintaten^  see  supra,  p.  33 J,  note. — Translator.] 
on  analysis,  to  be  simple^  but  before  analysis  it  §  [As  described  in  Hopkins,  Organ,  p.  142, 

is  to  us  only  a  (musical)  tone  like  any  other,  these  are  the  sesquialtera  *  of  five,  four,  three, 

and  hence  in  this  case  the  Author's  Klang  or   two  ranks  of    open    metal    pipes,    tuned 

becomes  the  Author's  Ton,    I  believe  that  the  in  Thirds,  Fifths,  and  Octaves  to  the  Diapa- 

language  used  in  my  translation  is  best  adapted  son.'     The  mixture,  consisting  of  five  to  two 

for  the  constant  accurate  distinction  between  ranks  of  open  metal  pipes  smaller  than  the 

compound  and  simple  tones  by  English  readers,  last,  is  in  England  the  second,  in  Germany  the 

u  I  leave  nothing  which  runs  counter  to  old  first,  compound  stop  (p.  143).  The  Furniture  ot 

habits,  and  by  the  use  of  the  words  simple  and  five  to  two  sets  of  small  open  pipes,  is  variable, 

compound,  constantly  recall  attention  to  this  i)  The  Comet,  mounted  has  five  ranks  of  very 

newly  discovered  and  extremely  important  rela-  large  and  loudly  voiced  pipes,  2)  the  echo,  ia 

tion.— Tran^Zator.]  similar,  but  light  and  delicate,  and  is  inclosed 

t  [Principal — double  open  diapason.  Oross-  in  a  box.    In  German  organs  the  comet  is  also 

96(2acfct— double  stopped  diapason.     Hopkins,  a  pedal  reed  stop  of  four  and  two  feet  (ibid.). — 

Orqan,  p.  444-  ^.—Translator.']  Translator."] 


X  [*  Geigen    Principal— violin     or    crisp- 


Digitized  by 


Google 


S8  DIFFICULTIES  IN  OBSERVING  PARTIALS.  pabt  i. 

The  nature  of  the  case  at  the  same  time  fully  justifies  their  use.  The  musician  is 
bound  to  regard  the  tones  of  all  musical  instruments  as  compounded  in  the  same 
way  as  the  compound  stops  of  organs,  and  the  important  part  this  method  of  com- 
position plays  in  the  construction  of  musical  scales  and  chords  will  be  made  evident 
in  subsequent  chapters. 

We  have  thus  been  led  to  an  appreciation  of  upper  partial  tones,  which  differs 
considerably  from  that  previously  entertained  by  musicians,  and  even  physicists, 
and  must  therefore  be  prepared  to  meet  the  opposition  which  will  be  raised.  The 
upper  partial  tones  were  indeed  known,  but  almost  only  in  such  compound  tones  as 
those  of  strings,  where  there  was  a  favourable  opportunity  for  observing  them ; 
but  they  appear  in  previous  physical  and  musical  works  as  an  isolated  accidental 
phenomenon  of  small  intensity,  a  kind  of  curiosity,  which  was  certainly  occasion- 
ally adduced,  in  order  to  give  some  support  to  the  opinion  that  nature  had  pre- 

If  figured  the  construction  of  our  major  chord,  but  which  on  the  whole  remained 
almost  entirely  disregarded.  In  opposition  to  this  we  have  to  assert,  and  we  shall 
prove  the  assertion  in  the  next  chapter,  that  upper  partial  tones  are,  with  a  few 
exceptions  already  named,  a  general  constituent  of  all  musical  tones,  and  that  a 
certain  stock  of  upper  partials  is  an  essential  condition  for  a  good  musical  quality 
of  tone.  Finally,  these  upper  partials  have  been  erroneously  considered  as  weak, 
because  they  are  difficult  to  observe,  while,  in  point  of  fact,  for  some  of  the  best 
musical  qualities  of  tone,  the  loudness  of  the  first  upper  partials  is  not  far  inferior 
to  that  of  the  prime  tone  itself. 

There  is  no  difficulty  in  verifying  this  last  fact  by  experiments  on  the  tones  of 
strings.  Strike  the  string  of  a  piano  or  monochord,  and  immediately  touch  one  of 
its  nodes  for  an  instant  with  the  finger ;  the  constituent  partial  tones  having  this 
node  will  remain  with  unaltered  loudness,  and  the  rest  will  disappear.  We  might 
also  touch  the  node  in  the  same  way  at  the  instant  of  striking,  and  thus  obtain  the 

f  corresponding  constituent  partial  tones  from  the  first,  in  place  of  the  complete 
compound  tone  of  the  note.  In  both  ways  we  can  readily  convince  ourselves  that  the 
first  upper  partials,  as  the  Octave  and  Twelfth,  are  by  no  means  weak  and  difiicult 
to  hear,  but  have  a  very  appreciable  strength.  In  some  cases  we  are  able  to  assign 
numerical  values  for  the  intensity  of  the  upper  partial  tones,  as. will  be  shewn  in 
the  next  chapter.  For  tones  not  produced  on  strings  this  d  posteriori  proof  is  not 
so  easy  to  conduct,  because  we  are  not  able  to  make  the  upper  partials  speak 
separately.  But  even  then  by  means  of  the  resonator  we  can  appreciate  the  in- 
tensity of  these  upper  partials  by  producing  the  corresponding  note  on  the  same 
or  some  other  instrument  until  its  loudness,  when  heard  through  the  resonator, 
agrees  with  that  of  the  former. 

The  difiiculty  we  experience  in  hearing  upper  partial  tones  is  no  reason  for 
considering  them  to  be  weak ;  for  this  difficulty  does  not  depend  on  their  intensity, 
but  upon  entirely  different  circumstances,  which  could  not  be  properly  estimated 

%  mitil  the  advances  recently  made  in  the  physiology  of  the  senses.  On  this  diffi- 
culty of  observing  the  upper  partial  tones  have  been  founded  the  objections  which 
A.  Seebeck  ♦  has  advanced  against  Ohm's  law  of  the  decomposition  of  a  musical 
tone  ;  and  perhaps  many  of  my  readers  who  are  unacquainted  with  the  physiology 
of  the  other  senses,  particularly  with  that  of  the  eye,  might  be  inclined  to  adopt 
Seebeck's  opinions.  I  am  therefore  obliged  to  enter  into  some  details  concerning 
this  difference  of  opinion,  and  the  peculiarities  of  the  perceptions  of  our  senses, 
on  which  the  solution  of  the  difficulty  depends. 

Seebeck,  although  extremely  accomplished  in  acoustical  experiments  and 
observations,  was  not  always  able  to  recognise  upper  partial  tones,  where  Ohm's 
law  required  them  to  exist.  But  we  are  also  bound  to  add  that  he  did  not  apply 
the  methods  already  indicated  for  directing  the  attention  of  his  ear  to  the  upper 
partials  in  question.    In  other  cases  when  he  did  hear  the  theoretical  upper 

*  In  Poggendorff's  Annalen  der  Physik,  vol.  Ix.  p.  449,  vol.  Ixiii.  pp.  353  and  368.— OAm, 
ibid,  vol.  lix.  p.  513,  and  vol.  Ixii.  p.  i. 

Digitized  by  VjOOQlC 


CHAP.  IV.         DIFFICULTIES  IN  OBSERVING  PARTIALS.  59 

paiiials,  they  were  weaker  than  the  theory  required.  He  concluded  that  the  defi- 
nition of  a  simple  tone  as  given  by  Ohm  was  too  limited,  and  that  not  only  pen- 
dular  vibrations,  but  other  vibrational  forms,  provided  they  were  not  too  widely 
separated  from  the  pendular,  were  capable  of  exciting  in  the  ear  the  sensation  of 
a  single  simple  tone,  which,  however,  had  a  variable  quality.  He  consequently 
asserted  that  when  a  musical  tone  was  compounded  of  several  simple  tones,  part 
of  the  intensity  of  the  upper  constituent  tones  went  to  increase  the  intensity  of 
the  prime  tone,  with  which  it  fused,  and  that  at  most  a  small  remainder  excited  in 
the  ear  the  sensation  of  an  upper  partial  tone.  He  did  not  formulate  any  deter- 
minate law,  assigning  the  vibrational  forms  which  would  give  the  impression  of 
a  simple  and  those  which  would  give  the  impression  of  a  compound  tone.  The 
experiments  of  Seebeck,  on  which  he  founded  his  assertions,  need  not  be  here 
described  in  detail.  Their  object  was  only  to  produce  musical  tones  for  which 
either  the  intensity  of  the  simple  vibrations  corresponding  to  the  upper  partials  % 
could  be  theoretically  calculated,  or  in  which  these  upper  partials  could  be 
rendered  separately  audible.  For  the  latter  purpose  the  siren  was  used.  We  have 
just  described  how  the  same  object  can  be  attained  by  means  of  strings.  Seebeck 
shews  in  each  case  that  the  simple  vibrations  corresponding  to  the  upper  partials 
have  considerable  strength,  but  that  the  upper  partials  are  either  not  heard  at  all, 
or  heard  with  difficulty  in  the  compound  tone  itself.  This  fact  has  been  already 
mentioned  in  the  present  chapter.  It  may  be  perfectly  true  for  an  observer  who 
has  not  appUed  the  proper  means  for  observing  upper  partials,  while  another,  or 
even  the  first  observer  himself  when  properly  assisted,  can  hear  them  perfectly  well.* 
Now  there  are  many  circumstances  which  assist  us  first  in  separating  the 
musical  tones  arising  from  different  sources,  and  secondly,  in  keeping  together  the 
partial  tones  of  each  separate  source.  Thus  when  one  musical  tone  is  heard  for 
some  time  before  being  joined  by  the  second,  and  then  the  second  continues  after 
the  first  has  ceased,  the  separation  in  sound  is  facilitated  by  the  succession  of  time.  ^ 
We  have  already  heard  the  first  musical  tone  by  itself,  and  hence  know  imme- 
diately what  we  have  to  deduct  from  the  compound  effect  for  the  effect  of  this  first 
tone.  Even  when  several  parts  proceed  in  the  same  rhythm  in  polyphonic  music, 
the  mode  in  which  the  tones  of  different  instruments  and  voices  commence,  the 
nature  of  their  increase  in  force,  the  certainty  with  which  they  are  held,  and  the 
manner  in  which  they  die  off,  are  generally  slightly  different  for  each.  Thus  the 
tones  of  a  pianoforte  commence  suddenly  with  a  blow,  and  are  consequently 
strongest  at  the  first  moment,  and  then  rapidly  decrease  in  power.  The  tones  of 
brass  instruments,  on  the  other  hand,  commence  sluggishly,  and  require  a  small 
but  sensible  time  to  develop  their  full  strength.  The  tones  of  bowed  instruments 
are  distinguished  by  their  extreme  mobility,  but  when  either  the  player  or  the 
instrument  is  not  unusually  perfect  they  are  interrupted  by  little,  very  short, 
pauses,  producing  in  the  ear  the  sensation  of  scraping,  as  will  be  described  more 
in  detail  when  we  come  to  analyse  the  musical  tone  of  a  violin.  When,  then,  such  ^f 
instruments  are  sounded  together  there  are  generally  points  of  time  when  one  or 
the  other  is  predominant,  and  it  is  consequently  easily  distinguished  by  the  ear. 
But  besides  all  this,  in  good  part  music,  especial  care  is  taken  to  facihtate  the 
separation  of  the  parts  by  the  ear.  In  polyphonic  music  proper,  where  each  part 
has  its  own  distinct  melody,  a  principal  means  of  clearly  separating  the  progres- 
sion of  each  part  has  always  consisted  in  making  them  proceed  in  different  rhythms 
and  on  different  divisions  of  the  bars  ;  or  where  this  could  not  be  done,  or  was  at 
any  rate  only  partly  possible,  as  in  four-part  chorales,  it  is  an  old  rule,  contrived 
for  this  purpose,  to  let  three  parts,  if  possible,  move  by  single  degrees  of  the  scale, 
and  let  the  fourth  leap  over  several.  The  small  amount  of  alteration  in  the  pitch 
makes  it  easier  for  the  listener  to  keep  the  identity  of  the  several  voices  distinctly 
in  mind. 

^*  [Here  from  *  Upper  partial  tones,*  p.  94,  to  *  former  analysis,*  p.  100  of  the  ist  English 
edition  are  omitted,  in  accordance  with  the  4th  German  edition. — Translator,] 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


6o 


FUSION  OF  PARTIALS  INTO  A  COMPOUND. 


PABT  I. 


Fm.  s 


AH  these  helps  £a.il  in  the  resolution  of  musical  tones  into  their  constituent 
partials.  When  a  compound  tone  commences  to  sound,  all  its  partial  tones 
commence  with  the  same  comparative  strength;  when  it  swells,  all  of  them 
generally  swell  uniformly ;  when  it  ceases,  all  cease  simultaneously.  Hence  no 
opportunity  is  generally  given  for  hearing  them  separately  and  independently.  In 
precisely  the  same  manner  as  the  naturally  connected  partial  tones  form  a  single 
source  of  sound,  the  partial  tones  in  a  compound  stop  on  the  organ  fuse  into  one,  as 
all  are  struck  with  the  same  digital,  and  all  move  in  the  same  melodic  progression 
as  their  prime  tone. 

Moreover,  the  tones  of  most  instruments  are  usually  accompanied  by  charac- 
teristic irregular  noises,  as  the  scratching  and  rubbing  of  the  violin  bow,  the  rush 
of  wind  in  flutes  and  organ  pipes,  the  grating  of  reeds,  &c.  These  noises,  with 
which  we  are  already  familiar  as  characterising  the  instruments,  materially 
H  facilitate  our  power  of  distinguishing  them  in  a  composite  mass  of  sounds.  The 
partial  tones  in  a  compound  have,  of  course,  no  such  characteristic  marks. 

Hence  we  have  no  reason  to  be  surprised  that  the  resolution  of  a  compound 
tone  into  its  partials  is  not  quite  so  easy  for  the  ear  to  accomplish,  as  the  resolu- 
tion of  composite  masses  of  the  musical  sounds  of  many  instruments  into  their 
proximate  constituents,  and  that  even  a  trained  musical  ear  requires  the  applica- 
tion of  a  considerable  amount  of  attention  when  it  undertakes  the  former  problem. 

It  is  easy  to  see  that  the  auxiliary  circumstances  already  named  do  not  always 
suffice  for  a  correct  separation  of  musical  tones.  In  imiformly  sustained  musical 
tones,  where  one  might  be  considered  as  an  upper  partial  of  another,  our 
judgment  might  readily  make  default.  This  is  reaUy  the  case.  G.  S.  Ohm 
proposed  a  very  instructive  experiment  to  shew  this,  using  the  tones  of  a  violin. 
But  it  is  more  suitable  for  such  an  experiment  to  use  simple  tones,  as  those  of  a 
stopped  organ  pipe.  The  best  instrument,  however,  is  a  glass  bottle  of  the  form 
H  shewn  in  fig.  20,  which  is  easily  procured  and 
prepared  for  the  experiment.  A  Uttle  rod  c 
supports  a  guttapercha  tube  a  in  a  proper 
position.  The  end  of  the  tube,  which  is 
directed  towards  the  bottle,  is  softened  in  warm 
water  and  pressed  flat,  forming  a  narrow  chink, 
through  which  air  can  be  made  to  rush  over 
the  mouth  of  the  bottle.  When  the  tube  is 
fastened  by  an  india-rubber  pipe  to  the  nozzle 
of  a  bellows,  and  wind  is  driven  over  the  bottle, 
it  produces  a  hollow  obscure  soimd,  like  the 
vowel  00  in  too,  which  is  freer  from  upper 
partial  tones  than  even  the  tone  of  a  stopped 
pipe,  and  is  only  accompanied  by  a  slight 
H  noise  of  wind.  I  find  that  it  is  easier  to  keep 
the  pitch  unaltered  in  this  instrument  while 
the  pressure  of  the  wind  is  slightly  changed, 
than  in  stopped  pipes.  We  deepen  the  tone  by 
partially  shading  the  orifice  of  the  bottle  with 
a  little  wooden  plate;  and  we  sharpen  it  by 
pouring  in  oil  or  melted  wax.  We  are  thus  able  to  make  any  required  little 
alterations  in  pitch.  I  tuned  a  large  bottle  to  bj;}  and  a  smaller  one  to  b^  and 
united  them  with  the  same  bellows,  so  that  when  used  both  began  to  speak  at  the 
same  instant.  When  thus  united  they  gave  a  musical  tone  of  the  pitch  of  the 
deeper  bj;},  but  having  the  quahty  of  tone  of  the  vowel  oa  in  toad,  instead  of  00  in 
too.  When,  then,  I  compressed  first  one  of  the  india-rubber  tubes  and  then  the 
other,  so  as  to  produce  the  tones  alternately,  separately,  and  in  connection,  I  was 
at  last  able  to  hear  them  separately  when  sounded  together,  but  I  could  not 
continue  to  hear  them  separately  for  long,  for  the  upper  tone  gradually  fused  with 

Digitized  by^OOQlC 


CHAP.  nr.  SEPAEATION  OF  THE   PABTIALS.  6i 

the  lower.  This  fusion  takes  place  even  when  the  upper  tone  is  somewhat  stronger 
than  the  lower.  The  alteration  in  the  quality  of  tone  which  takes  place  during 
this  fusion  is  characteristic.  On  producing  the  upper  tone  first  and  then  letting 
the  lower  sound  with  it,  I  found  that  I  at  first  continued  to  hear  the  upper  tone 
with  its  full  force,  and  the  under  tone  sounding  helow  it  in  its  natural  quality  of 
00  in  too.  But  by  degrees,  as  my  recollection  of  the  sound  of  the  isolated  upper 
tone  died  away,  it  seemed  to  become  more  and  more  indistinct  and  weak,  while 
the  lower  tone  appeared  to  become  stronger,  and  sounded  like  oa  in  toad.  This 
weakening  of  the  upper  and  strengthening  of  the  lower  tone  was  also  observed  by 
Ohm  on  the  violin.  As  Seebeck  remarks,  it  certainly  does  not  always  occur,  and 
probably  depends  on  the  liveliness  of  our  recollections  of  the  tones  as  heard 
separately,  and  the  greater  or  less  uniformity  in  the  simultaneous  production  of 
the  tones.  But  where  the  experiment  succeeds,  it  gives  the  best  proof  of  the 
essential  dependence  of  the  result  on  varying  activity  of  attention.  With  the  tones  If 
produced  by  bottles,  in  addition  to  the  reinforcement  of  the  lower  tone,  the  altera- 
tion in  its  quality  is  very  evident  and  is  characteristic  of  the  nature  of  the  process. 
This  alteration  is  less  striking  for  the  penetrating  tones  of  the  violin.* 

This  experiment  has  been  appealed  to  both  by  Ohm  and  by  Seebeck  as  a 
corroboration  of  their  different  opinions.  When  Ohm  stated  that  it  was  an 
'  illusion  of  the  ear '  to  apprehend  the  upper  partial  tones  wholly  or  partly  as  a 
reinforcement  of  the  prime  tone  (or  rather  of  the  compound  tone  whose  pitch  is 
determined  by  that  of  its  prime),  he  certainly  used  a  somewhat  incorrect  expression, 
although  he  meant  what  was  correct,  and  Seebeck  was  justified  in  replying  that 
the  ear  was  the  sole  judge  of  auditory  sensations,  and  that  the  mode  in  which  it 
apprehended  tones  ought  not  to  be  called  an  *  illusion.'  However,  our  experiments 
just  described  shew  that  the  judgment  of  the  ear  differs  according  to  the  liveliness 
of  its  recollection  of  the  separate  auditory  impressions  here  fused  into  one  whole, 
and  according  to  the  intensity  of  its  attention.  Hence  we  can  certainly  appeal  from  % 
the  sensations  of  an  ear  directed  without  assistance  to  external  objects,  whose 
interests  Seebeck  represents,  to  the  ear  which  is  attentively  observing  itself  and 
is  suitably  assisted  in  its  observation.  Such  an  ear  really  proceeds  according  to 
the  law  laid  down  by  Ohm. 

Another  experiment  should  be  adduced.  Eaise  the  dampers  of  a  pianoforte  so 
that  all  the  strings  can  vibrate  freely,  then  sing  the  vowel  a  ia  father,  art,  loudly 
to  any  note  of  the  piano,  directing  the  voice  to  the  sounding  board ;  the  sym- 
pathetic resonance  of  the  strings  distinctly  re-echoes  the  same  a.  On  singing  oe 
in  toe,  the  same  oe  is  re-echoed.  On  singing  a  in  fare,  this  a  is  re-echoed.  For  ee 
in  see  the  echo  is  not  quite  so  good.  The  experiment  does  not  succeed  so  well  if 
the  damper  is  removed  only  from  the  note  on  which  the  vowels  are  sung.  The 
vowel  character  of  the  echo  arises  from  the  re-echoing  of  those  upper  partial  tones 
which  characterise  the  vowels.  These,  however,  will  echo  better  and  more 
clearly  when  their  corresponding  higher  strings  are  free  and  can  vibrate  sym-  ^ 
pathetically.  In  this  case,  then,  in  the  last  resort,  the  musical  effect  of  the 
resonance  is  compounded  of  the  tones  of  several  strings,  and  several  separate 
partial  tones  combine  to  produce  a  musical  tone  of  a  peculiar  quality.  In  addition 
to  the  vowels  of  the  human  voice,  the  piano  will  also  quite  distinctly  imitate  the 
quality  of  tone  produced  by  a  clarinet,  when  strongly  blown  on  to  the  sounding 
board. 

Finally,  we  must  remark,  that  although  the  pitch  of  a  compound  tone  is,  for 

*   [A  very  convenient  form  of  this  ezpezi-  The  tone  is  also  brighter  and  unaccompanied 

mont,  useful  even  for  lecture  purposes,  is  to  by  any  windrush.    By  pressing  the  handle  of 

employ  two  tuning-forks,  tuned  as  an  Octave,  the  deeper  fork  on  the  table,  we  can  excite  its 

say  &  and  c",  and  held  over  separate  resonance  other  upper  partials,  and  thus  produce  a  third 

jars.    By  removing  first  one  and  then  the  other,  quality  of  tone,  which  can  be  readily  appre- 

or  letting  both  sound  together,  the  above  effects  ciated ;  thus,  simple  c',  simple  cf  +  simple  c", 

can  be  made  evident,  and  they  even  remain  compound  c'. — Translator,] 
when  the  Octave  is  not  tuned  perfectly  true. 


Digitized  by 


Google 


62  ANALYSIS  OP  COMPOUND   SENSATIONS.  pabt  i. 

musical  purposes,  determined  by  that  of  its  prime,  the  influence  of  the  upper 
partial  tones  is  by  no  means  unfelt.  They  give  the  compound  tone  a  brighter  and 
higher  effect.  Simple  tones  are  dull.  When  they  are  compared  with  compound 
tones  of  the  same  pitch,  we  are  inclined  to  estimate  the  compound  as  belonging  to 
a  higher  Octave  than  the  simple  tones.  The  difference  is  of  the  same  kind  as  that 
heard  when  first  the  vowel  oo  in  too  and  then  a  in  tar  are  sung  to  the  same  note. 
It  is  often  extremely  difficult  to  compare  the  pitches  of  compound  tones  of  different 
qualities.  It  is  very  easy  to  make  a  mistake  of  an  Octave.  This  has  happened 
to  the  most  celebrated  musicians  and  acousticians.  Thus  it  is  well  known  that 
Tartini,  who  was  celebrated  as  a  violinist  and  theoretical  musician,  estimated  all 
combinational  tones  (Chap.  XI.)  an  Octave  too  high,  and,  on  the  other  hand, 
Henrici  *  assigns  a  pitch  too  low  by  an  Octave  to  the  upper  partial  tones  of 
tuning-forks.f 

I  The  problem  to  be  solved,  then,  in  distinguishing  the  partials  of  a  compound 
tone  is  that  of  analysing  a  given  aggregate  of  sensations  into  elements  which  no 
longer  admit  of  analysis.  We  are  accustomed  in  a  large  number  of  cases  where 
sensations  of  different  kinds  or  in  different  parts  of  the  body,  exist  simultaneously, 
to  recognise  that  they  are  distinct  as  soon  as  they  are  perceived,  and  to  direct  our 
attention  at  will  to  any  one  of  them  separately.  Thus  at  any  moment  we  can  be 
separately  conscious  of  what  we  see,  of  what  we  hear,  of  what  we  feel,  and  dis- 
tinguish what  we  feel  in  a  finger  or  in  the  great  toe,  whether  pressure  or  a  gentle 
touch,or  warmth.  So  also  in  the  field  of  vision.  Indeed,  as  I  shall  endeavour  to  shew 
in  what  follows,  we  readily  distinguish  our  sensations  from  one  another  when  we 
have  a  precise  knowledge  that  they  are  composite,  as,  for  example,  when  we  have 
become  certain,  by  frequently  repeated  and  invariable  experience,  that  our  present 
sensation  arises  from  the  simultaneous  action  of  many  independent  stimuli,  each 
of  which  usually  excites  an  equally  well-known  individual  sensation.     This  induces 

1  f  us  to  think  that  nothing  can  be  easier,  when  a  number  of  different  sensations  are 

,     simultaneously  excited,  than  to  distinguish  them  individually  from  each  other,  and 

1     that  this  is  an  innate  faculty  of  our  minds. 

Thus  we  find,  among  others,  that  it  is  quite  a  matter  of  course  to  hear  sepa- 
rately the  different  musical  tones  which  come  to  our  senses  collectively,  and  expect 
that  in  every  case  when  two  of  them  occur  together,  we  shall  be  able  to  do  the 
like. 

The  matter  is  very  different  when  we  set  to  work  at  investigating  the  more  un- 
usual cases  of  perception,  and  at  more  completely  understanding  the  conditions  under 
which  the  above-mentioned  distinction  can  or  cannot  be  made,  as  is  the  case  in  the 
physiology  of  the  senses.  We  then  become  aware  that  two  different  kinds  or  grades 
must  be  distinguished  in  our  becoming  conscious  of  a  sensation.  The  lower  grade  of 
this  consciousness,  is  that  where  the  influence  of  the  sensation  in  question  makes 
itself  felt  only  in  the  conceptions  we  form  of  external  things  and  processes,  and  assists 

^  in  determining  them.  This  can  take  place  without  our  needing  or  indeed  being  able 
to  ascertain  to  what  particular  part  of  our  sensations  we  owe  this  or  that  relation 
of  our  perceptions.  In  this  case  we  will  say  that  the  impression  of  the  sensation  in 
question  is  perceived  synthetically.  The  second  and  higher  grade  is  when  we 
immediately  distinguish  the  sensation  in  question  as  an  existing  part  of  the  sum 
of  the  sensations  excited  in  us.  We  will  say  then  that  the  sensation  is  perceived 
analytically .X    The  two  cases  must  be  carefully  distinguished  firom  each  other. 

♦  Poggd.    Ann.,    vol.    xcix.  p.  506.     The  with  wahrgenommeny  and  then  restricting  the 

same  difficulty  is    mentioned  by   Zamminer  meaning  of  this  very  common  German  word. 

{Die   Musik  und  die  musikalischen  Instru-  It  appeared  to  me  that  it  would  be  clearer  to 

merUe,  1855,  p.  1 11)  as  well  known  to  musicians.  an   English  reader  not  to  invent  new  words 

f  [Here  the  passage  from  '  The  problem  or  restrict  the  sense  of   old  words,   but    to 

to  be  solved,*  p.  626,  to    *  from  its    simple  use  perceived  in  both  cases,  and  distinguish 

tones,*  p.  656,  is  inserted  in  this  edition  from  the  them  (for  percipirt  and  apperdpirt  respectively) 

4th  German  edition.— TraTt^Za^r.]  by  the  adjuncts  synthetically  and  analytically^ 

i  [Prof.  Helmholtz  uses  Leibnitz's  terms  the  use  of  which  is  clear  from  the  explanations 

percipirt  and  apperdpirt ^  alternating  the  latter  given  in  the  iBxt,— Translator.] 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


CHAP.  IV.  ANALYSIS  OP  COMPOUND  SENSATIONS.  63 

Seebeck  and  Ohm  are  agreed  that  the  upper  partials  of  a  musical  tone  are 
perceiyed  synthetically.  This  is  acknowledged  by  Seebeck  when  he  admits  that 
their  action  on  the  ear  changes  the  force  or  quality  of  the  sound  examined.  The 
dispute  turns  upon  whether  in  all  cases  they  can  be  perceived  analytically  in  their 
individual  existence ;  that  is,  whether  the  ear  when  unaided  by  resonators  or  other 
physical  auxiliaries,  which  themselves  alter  the  mass  of  musical  sound  heard  by  the 
observer,  can  by  mere  direction  and  intensity  of  attention  distinguish  whether,  and 
if  so  in  what  force,  the  Octave,  the  Twelfth,  &c ,  of  the  prime  exists  in  the  given 
mnsical  sound. 

In  the  first  place  I  will  adduce  a  series  of  examples  which  shew  that  the 
difficulty  felt  in  analysing  musical  tones  exists  also  for  other  senses.  Let  us 
begin  with  the  comparatively  simple  perceptions  of  the  sense  of  taste.  The 
ingredients  of  our  dishes  and  the  spices  with  which  we  flavour  them,  are  not  so 
complicated  that  they  could  not  be  readily  learned  by  any  one.  And  yet  there  are  if 
very  few  people  who  have  not  themselves  practically  studied  cookery,  that  are  able 
readily  and  correctly  to  discover,  by  the  taste  alone,  the  ingredients  of  the  dishes 
placed  before  them.  How  much  practice,  and  perhaps  also  peculiar  talent,  belongs 
to  wine  tasting  for  the  purpose  of  discovering  adulterations  is  known  in  all  wme- 
growing  countries.  Similarly  for  smell ;  indeed  the  sensations  of  taste  and  smell 
may  unite  to  form  a  single  whole.  Using  our  tongues  constantly,  we  are  scarcely 
aware  that  the  peculiar  character  of  many  articles  of  food  and  drink,  as  vinegar  or 
wine,  depends  also  upon  the  sensation  of  smell,  their  vapours  entering  the  back 
part  of  the  nose  tlirough  the  gullet.  It  is  not  till  we  meet  vdth  persons  in  whom 
the  sense  of  smell  is  deficient  that  we  learn  how  essential  a  part  it  plays  in 
tasting.  Such  persons  are  constantly  in  fault  when  judging  of  food,  as  mdeed  any 
one  can  learn  from  his  own  experience,  when  he  suffers  from  a  heavy  cold  in  the 
head  without  having  a  loaded  tongue. 

When  our  hand  glides  unawares  along  a  cold  and  smooth  piece  of  metal  we  % 
are  apt  to  imagine  that  we  have  wetted  our  hand.  This  shews  that  the  sensation 
of  witness  to  the  touch  is  compounded  out  of  that  of  unresisting  ghding  and  cold, 
which  in  one  case  results  from  the  good  heat-conducting  properties  of  metal,  and 
in  the  other  from  the  cold  of  evaporation  and  the  great  specific  heat  of  water. 
We  can  easily  recognise  both  sensations  in  wetness,  when  we  think  over  tlie 
matter,  but  it  is  the  above-mentioned  illusion  which  teaches  us  that  the  peculiar 
feeling  of  wetness  is  entirely  resolvable  into  these  two  sensations. 

The  discovery  of  the  stereoscope  has  taught  us  that  the  power  of  seeing  the 
depths  of  a  field  of  view,  that  is,  the  different  distances  at  which  objects  and 
their  parts  lie  from  the  eye  of  the  spectator,  essentially  depends  on  the  simul- 
taneous synthetical  perceptions  of  two  somewhat  different  perspective  images  of 
the  same  objects  by  the  two  eyes  of  the  observer.  If  the  difference  of  the  two 
images  is  sufficiently  great  it  is  not  difficult  to  perceive  them  analytically  as 
separate.  For  example,  if  we  look  intently  at  a  distant  object  and  hold  one  of  H 
our  fingers  slightly  in  front  of  our  nose  we  see  two  images  of  our  finger  against 
the  background,  one  of  which  vanishes  when  we  close  the  right  eye,  the  other 
belonging  to  the  left.  But  when  the  differences  of  distance  are  relatively  small, 
and  hence  the  differences  of  the  two  perspective  images  on  the  retina  are  so  also, 
great  practice  and  certainty  in  the  observation  of  double  images  is  necessary  to 
keep  them  asnnder,  yet  the  synthetical  perception  of  their  differences  still  exists, 
and  makes  itself  felt  in  the  apparent  relief  of  the  surface  viewed.  In  this  case 
also,  as  well  as  for  upper  partial  tones,  the  ease  and  exactness  of  the  analytical 
perception  is  far  behind  that  of  the  synthetical  perception. 

In  the  conception  which  we  form  of  the  direction  in  which  the  objects  viewed 
fieem  to  he,  a  considerable  part  must  be  played  by  those  sensations,  mainly  muscular, 
which  enable  us  to  recognise  the  position  of  our  body,  of  the  head  with  regard  to 
the  body,  and  of  the  eye  with  regard  to  the  head.  If  one  of  these  is  altered,  for 
example,  if  the  sensation  of  the  proper  position  of  the  eye  is  changed  by  pressing  t 

Digitized  by  VjOOQ  IC 


64  ANALYSIS  OF  COMPOUND   SENSATIONS.  pabt  l 

a  finger  against  the  eyeball  or  by  injury  to  one  of  the  muscles  of  the  eye,  our  per- 
ception of  the  position  of  visible  objects  is  also  changed.  But  it  is  only  by  such 
occasional  illusions  that  we  become  aware  of  the  fact  that  muscular  sensations  form 
part  of  the  aggregate  of  sensations  by  which  our  conception  of  the  position  of  a 
visible  object  is  determined. 
t^Vrv  The  phenomena  of  mixed  colours  present  considerable  analogy  to  those  of  com- 

Jl^wjiaWI  P^^'i^d  musical  tones,  only  in  the  case  of  colour  the  number  of  sensations  reduces  to 
^^'^^  three,  and  the  analysis  of  the  composite  sensations  into  their  simple  elements  is  still 
more  difficult  and  imperfect  than  for  musical  tones.  As  early  as  1686  R.  Waller 
mentions  in  the  Philosophical  Transactions  the  reduction  of  all  colours  to  the 
mixture  of  three  fundamental  colours,  as  something  already  well  known.  This 
view  could  in  earlier  times  only  be  founded  on  sensations  and  experiments  arising 
from  the  mixture  of  pigments.    In  recent  times  we  have  discovered  better  methods, 

%  by  mixing  light  of  different  colours,  and  hence  have  confirmed  the  correctness  of 
that  hj^othesis  by  exact  measurements,  but  at  the  same  time  we  have  learned  that 
this  confirmation  only  succeeds  within  a  certain  limit,  conditioned  by  the  fact  that  no 
kind  of  coloured  light  exists  which  can  give  us  the  sensation  of  a  single  one  of  the 
fundamental  colours  with  exclusive  purity.  Even  the  most  saturated  and  purest 
colours  that  the  external  world  presents  to  us  in  the  prismatic  spectrum,  may  by 
the  development  of  secondary  images  of  the  complementary  colours  in  the  eye 
be  still  freed  as  it  were  from  a  white  veil,  and  hence  cannot  be  considered  as  abso- 
lutely pure.  For  tliis  reason  we  are  unable  to  shew  objectively  the  absolutely  pure 
fundamental  colours  from  a  mixture  of  which  all  other  colours  without  exception 
can  be  formed.  We  only  know  that  among  the  colours  of  the  spectrum  scarlet-red, 
yellow-green,  and  blue-violet  approach  to  them  nearer  than  any  other  objective 
colours.*  Hence  we  are  able  to  compound  out  of  these  three  colours  almost  all  the 
colours  that  usually  occur  in  different  natural  bodies,  but  we  cannot  produce  the 

^  yellow  and  blue  of  the  spectrum  in  that  complete  degree  of  saturation  which  they 
reach  when  purest  within  the  spectrum  itself.  Our  mixtures  are  always  a  little 
whiter  than  the  corresponding  simple  colours  of  the  spectrum.  Hence  it  follows 
that  we  never  see  the  simple  elements  of  our  sensations  of  colour,  or  at  least  see 
them  only  for  a  very  short  time  in  particular  experiments  directed  to  this  end,  and 
consequently  cannot  have  any  such  exact  or  certain  image  in  our  recollection,  as 
would  indisputably  be  necessary  for  accurately  analysing  every  sensation  of  colour 
into  its  elementary  sensations  by  inspection.  Moreover  we  have  relatively  rare 
opportunities  of  observing  the  process  of  the  composition  of  colours,  and  hence  of 
recognising  the  constituents  in  the  compound.  It  certainly  appears  to  me  very 
characteristic  of  this  process,  that  for  a  century  and  a  half,  from  Waller  to  Goethe, 
every  one  relied  on  the  mixtures  of  pigments,  and  hence  believed  green  to  be  a 
mixture  of  blue  and  yellow,  whereas  when  sky-blue  and  sulphur-yellow  beams  of 
Hght,  not  pigments,  are  mixed  together,  the  result  is  white.     To  this  very  cir- 

f  cumstance  is  due  the  violent  opposition  of  Goethe,  who  was  only  acquainted  with 
the  colours  of  pigments,  to  the  assertion  that  white  was  a  mixture  of  variously 
coloured  beams  of  hght.  Hence  we  can  have  Httle  doubt  that  the  power  of  dis- 
tinguishing the  different  elementary  constituents  of  the  sensation  is  originally 
absent  in  the  sense  of  sight,  and  that  the  little  which  exists  in  highly  educated 
observers,  has  been  attained  by  specially  conducted  experiments,  through  which  of 
course,  when  wrongly  planned,  error  may  have  ensued. 

On  the  other  hand  every  individual  has  an  opportunity  of  experimenting  on  the 

*  [In  his  Physiological  Optics^  p.  227,  E^^  hence  I  translate  span-griln  by  *  yellow- 
Prof.  Helmholtz  calls  scarlet-red  or  vermilion  green.'  Maxwell's  blae  or  third  colour  was 
the  part  of  the  spectrum  before  reaching  between  the  lines  F  and  Gy  but  twice  as  far 
Fraunhofer's  line  C  He  does  not  use  span-  from  the  latter  as  the  former.  This  gives  the 
grUnt  ( -  ChrUn-span  or  verdigris,  literally  colour  which  Prof.  H.  in  his  Optics  calls  *  cya- 
'  Spanish  green ')  in  his  Optics,  but  talks  of  nogen  blue,'  or  Prussian  blue.  The  violet 
green-yellow  between  the  lines  E  and  6,  and  proper  does  not  begin  till  after  the  line  O.  It 
he  says,  on  p.  844,  that  Maxwell  took  as  one  of  is  usual  to  speak  of  these  three  colours,  vaguely, 
the  fundamental  colours  *  a  green  near  the  line  as  Bed,  Green,  and  Blue.— Translator.] 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


CHAPS.  IV.  V.      ANALYSIS  OP  COMPOUND  SENSATIONS.  ^5 

composition  of  two  or  more  musical  sounds  or  noises  on  the- most  extended  scale, 
and  the  power  of  analysing  even  extremely  involved  compounds  of  musical  tones, 
into  the  separate  parts  produced  by  individual  instruments,  can  readily  be  acquired 
by  any  one  who  directs  his  attention  to  the  subject.  But  the  ultimate  simple 
elements  of  the  sensation  of  tone,  simple  tones  themselves,  are  rarely  heard  alone. 
Even  those  instruments  by  which  they  can  be  produced,  as  tuning-forks  before 
resonance  chambers,  when  strongly  excited,  give  rise  to  weak  harmonic  upper 
partials,  partly  within  and  partly  without  the  ear,  as  we  shall  see  in  Chapters  V. 
and  VII.  Hence  in  this  case  dso,  the  opportunities  are  very  scanty  for  impress- 
ing on  our  memory  an  exact  and  sure  image  of  ^ese  simple  elementary  tones. 
But  if  the  constituents  to  be  added  are  only  indefinitely  and  vaguely  known,  the 
analysis  of  the  sum  into  those  parts  must  be  correspondingly  uncertain.  If  we  do 
not  know  with  certainty  how  much  of  the  musical  tone  mider  consideration  is  to 
be  attributed  to  its  prime,  we  cannot  but  be  uncertain  as  to  what  belongs  to  the  ^ 
partials.  Consequently  we  must  begin  by  making  the  individual  elements  which 
have  to  be  distinguished,  individually  audible,  so  as  to  obtain  an  entirely  fresh 
recollection  of  the  corresponding  sensation,  and  the  whole  business  requires  un- 
disturbed and  concentrated  attention.  We  are  even  without  the  ease  that  can  be 
obtained  by  frequent  repetitions  of  the  experiment,  such  as  we  possess  in  the 
analysis  of  musical  chords  into  their  individual  tones.  In  that  case  we  hear  the 
individual  tones  sufficiently  often  by  themselves,  whereas  we  rarely  hear  simple 
tones  and  may  almost  be  said  never  to  hear  the  building  up  of  a  compound  from  its 
simple  tones. 

The  results  of  the  preceding  discussion  may  be  summed  up  as  follows  : — 

I.)  The  upper  partial  tones  corresponding  to  the  simple  vibrations  of  a  com- 
pound motion  of  the  air,  are  perceived  synthetically,  even  when  they  are  not  always 
perceived  analytically. 

2.)  But  they  can  be  made  objects  of  analytical  perception  without  any  other  If 
help  than  a  proper  direction  of  attention. 

3.)  Even  in  the  case  of  their  not  being  separately  perceived,  because  they  fuse 
into  the  whole  mass  of  musical  sound,  their  existence  in  our  sensation  is  established 
by  an  alteration  in  the  quality  of  tone,  the  impression  of  their  higher  pitch  being 
eharacteristically  marked  by  increased  brightness  and  acuteness  of  quality. 

In  the  next  chapter  we  shall  give  details  of  the  relations  of  the  upper  partials 
to  the  quality  of  compound  tones. 


CHAPTER  V. 

ON   THE   DIFFEBENCEB  IN   THE   QUALITY  OF  MUSICAL   TONES. 

TowABDS  the  close  of  Chapter  I.  (p.  21  j),  we  found  that  differences  in  the  quality 
of  musical  tones  must  depend  on  the  form  of  the  vibration  of  the  air.  The  T 
reasons  for  this  assertion  were  only  negative.  We  had  seen  that  force  depended 
on  amplitude,  and  pitch  on  rapidity  of  vibration  :  nothing  else  was  left  to  distin- 
guish quality  but  vibrational  form.  We  then  proceeded  to  shew  that  the  existence 
and  force  of  the  upper  partial  tones  which  accompanied  the  prime  depend  also  on 
the  vibrational  form,  and  hence  we  could  not  but  conclude  that  musical  tones  of 
the  same  quality  would  always  exhibit  the  same  combination  of  partials,  seeing 
that  the  peculiar  vibrational  form  which  excites  in  the  ear  the  sensation  of  a  certain 
quality  of  tone,  must  always  evoke  the  sensation  of  its  corresponding  upper  partials. 
The  question  then  arises,  can,  and  if  so,  to  what  extent  can  the  differences  of 
musical  quality  be  reduced  to  the  combination  of  different  partial  tones  with  dif- 
ferent intensities  in  different  musical  tones?  At  the  conclusion  of  last  chapter 
(p.  &od)j  we  saw  that  even  artificially  combined  simple  tones  were  capable  of  fusing 
into  a  musical  tone  of  a  quality  distinctly  different  from  that  of  either  of  its  con- 
stituents, and  that  consequently  the  existence  of  a  new  upper  partial  really  altered 

Digitized  by^DOQlC 


66  CONCEPTION  OF  MUSICAL  QUALITY  AND  TONE,     pabt  l 

the  quality  of  a  tone.    By  this  means  we  gained  a  clue  to  the  hitherto  enigmatical 
nature  of  quality  of  tone,  and  to  the  cause  of  its  varieties. 

There  has  been  a  general  inclination  to  credit  quaUty  with  all  possible  pecu- 
liarities of  musical  tones  that  were  not  evidently  due  to  force  and  pitch.  This  was 
correct  to  the  extent  that  quality  of  tone  was  merely  a  negative  conception.  But 
very  slight  consideration  will  suffice  to  shew  that  many  of  these  peculiarities  of 
musical  tones  depend  upon  the  way  in  which  they  begin  and  end.  The  methods  of 
attacking  and  releasing  tones  are  sometimes  so  characteristic  that  for  the  human 
voice  they  have  been  noted  by  a  series  of  different  letters.  To  these  belong  the  ex- 
plosive consonants  B,  D,  G,  and  P,  T,  E.  The  effects  of  these  letters  are  produced 
by  opening  the  closed,  or  closing  the  open  passage  through  the  mouth.  For  B 
and  P  the  closure  is  made  by  the  lips,  for  D  and  T  by  the  tongue  and  upper  teeth,* 
for  G  and  E  by  the  back  of  the  tongue  and  soft  palate.    The  series  of  the  mediae 

V  B,  D,  G  is  distinguished  from  that  of  the  tenues  P,  T,  E,  by  the  glottis  being  suffi- 
ciently narrowed,  when  the  closure  of  the  former  is  released,  to  produce  voice,  or  at 
least  the  rustle  of  whisper,  whereas  for  the  latter  or  termes  the  glottis  is  wide  open,t 
and  cannot  sound.  The  mediae  are  therefore  accompanied  by  voice,  which  is 
capable  of  commencing  at  the  beginning  of  a  syllable  an  instant  before  the  open- 
ing of  the  mouth,  and  of  lasting  at  the  end  of  a  syllable  a  moment  after  the  closure 
of  the  mouth,  because  some  air  can  be  still  driven  into  the  closed  cavity  of  the 
mouth  and  the  vibration  of  the  vocal  chords  in  the  larynx  can  be  still  maintained. 
On  account  of  the  narrowing  of  the  glottis  the  influx  of  air  is  more  moderate,  and 
the  noise  of  the  wind  less  sharp  for  the  mediae  than  the  tenueSy  which,  being  spoken 
with  open  glottis,  allow  of  a  great  deal  of  wind  being  forced  at  once  from  the  chest.t 
At  the  same  time  the  resonance  of  the  cavity  of  the  mouth,  which,  as  we  shall 
more  clearly  understand  further  on,  exercises  a  great  influence  on  the  vowels, 
varies  its  pitch,  corresponding  to  the  rapid  alterations  in  the  magnitude  of  its  volume 

f  and  oriflce,  and  this  brings  about  a  corresponding  rapid  variation  in  the  quality  of  the 
speech  sound. 

,  As  with  consonants,  the  differences  in  the  quality  of  tone  of  struck  strings, 
also  partly  depends  on  the  rapidity  with  which  the  tone  dies  away.  When  tlie 
strings  have  little  mass  (such  as  those  of  gut),  and  are  fEistened  to  a  very  mobile 
sounding  board  (as  for  a  violin,  guitar,  or  zither),  or  when  the  parts  on  which  they 
rest  or  which  they  touch  are  but  slightly  elastic  (as  when  the  violin  strings,  for 
example,  are  pressed  on  the  finger  board  by  the  soft  point  of  the  finger),  their 
vibrations  rapidly  disappear  after  striking,  and  the  tone  is  dry,  short,  and  without 
ring,  as  in  the  pizzicato  of  a  violin.  But  if  the  strings  are  of  metal  wire,  and 
hence  of  greater  weight  and  tension,  and  if  they  are  attached  to  strong  heavy 
bridges  which  cannot  be  much  shaken,  they  give  out  their  vibrations  slowly  to  the 

*  [This  is  true  for  German,  and  most  Con-  examples,  it  seemed  better  in  the  present  ease, 

tinental    languages,  and    for    some  dialectal  where  the  author  was  speaking  especially  of 

%  English,  especially  ^n  Cumberland,  Westmore-  the  phenomena  of  speech  to  which  he  waa 

land,  Yorkshire,  Lancashire,  the  Peak  of  Derby-  personidly  accustom^,  to  leave  the  text  un- 

shire,  and  Ireland,  but  even  then  only  in  con-  altered  and  draw  attention  to  English  peculiari- 

nection  with  the  trilled  B.    Throughout  Eng-  ties  in  footnotes. — Translator,] 

land  generally,  the  tip  of  the  tongue  is  quite  %  [Observe  again  that  this  description  of 

free  from  the  teeth,  except  for  TH  in  thin  and  the   rush   of   wind  accompanying  P,  T,  K, 

then,  and  for  T  and  D  it  only  touches  the  hard  although  true  for  German  habits  of  speech,  is 

palate,  seldom  advancing  so  far  as  the  root  of  not  true  for  the  usual  English  habits,  which 

the  gamB.^Translator,]  require  the  windrush  between  the  opening  of 

f  [This  again  is  true  for  German,  but  not  the  mouth  and  sounding  of  the  vowel  to  be 

for  English,  French,  or  Italian,  and  not  even  entirely  suppressed.    The  English  result  ia  a 

for  the  adjacent  Slavonic  languages.    In  these  gliding  vowel  sound  preceding  Uie  true  vowel  on 

languages  the  glottis  is  quite  closed  for  both  commencing  a  syllable,  and  following  the  vowel 

the  mediae  and  the  tenues  in  ordinary  speech,  on  ending  one.  The  difference  between  English 

but  the  voice  begins  for  the  mediae  before  P  and  German  Pis  precisely  the  same  (as  I  have 

releasing  the  closure  of  the  lips  or  tongue  and  verified  by  actual  observation)  as  that  between 

palate,  and  for  the  tenues  at  the  moment  of  the  simple  Sanscrit  tenuis  P,  and  the  postaspi. 

release.  Although  in  giving  vowel  sounds,  Ac,  rated  Sanscrit  Ph,  as  now  actually  pronounced 

I  have  generally  contented  myself  with  trans-  by  cultivated Bengalese.  Beemy  Early  English 

lating  the  same   into   English  symbols  and  Pronunciationt'p,  11^6^061,1,— Translator:] 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


CHAP.  V.    CONCEPTION  OF  MUSICAL  QUALITY  AND  TONE.  67 

air  and  the  sounding  board ;  their  vibrations  continue  longer,  their  tone  is  more 
durable  and  fuller,  as  in  the  pianoforte,  but  is  comparatively  less  powerful  and 
penetrating  than  that  of  gut  strings,  which  give  up  their  tone  more  readily  when 
struck  with  the  same  force.  Hence  the  pizzicato  of  bowed  instruments  when  well 
executed  is  much  more  piercing  than  the  tone  of  a  pianoforte.  Pianofortes  with 
their  strong  and  heavy  supports  for  the  strings  have,  consequently,  for  the  same 
thickness  of  string,  a  less  penetrating  but  a  much  more  lasting  tone  than  those 
instruments  of  which  the  supports  for  the  strings  are  lighter. 

It  is  very  characteristic  of  brass  instruments,  as  trumpets  and  trombones, 
that  their  tones  commence  abruptly  and  sluggishly.  The  various  tones  in  these 
instruments  are  produced  by  exciting  different  upper  partials  through  different 
styles  of  blowing,  which  serve  to  throw  the  column  of  air  into  vibrating  portions 
of  different  numbers  and  lengths  similar  to  those  on  a  string.  It  always  requires 
a  certain  amount  of  effort  to  excite  the  new  condition  of  vibration  in  place  of  the  ^ 
old,  but  when  once  established  it  is  maintained  with  less  exertion.  On  the  other 
hand,  the  transition  from  one  tone  to  another  is  easy  for  wooden  wind  instruments, 
as  the  flute,  oboe,  and  clarinet,  where  the  length  of  the  colmnn  of  air  is  readily 
changed  by  application  of  the  fingers  to  the  side  holes  and  keys,  and  where  the 
style  of  blowing  has  not  to  be  materially  altered. 

These  examples  will  suffice  to  shew  how  certain  characteristic  peculiarities  in 
the  tones  of  several  instruments  depend  on  the  mode  in  which  they  begin  and  end. 
When  we  speak  in  what  follows  of  musical  quaUty  of  tone,  we  shall  disregard 
these  peculiarities  of  beginning  and  ending,  and  confine  our  attention  to  the 
peculiarities  of  the  musical  tone  which  continues  uniformly. 

But  even  when  a  musical  tone  continues  with  uniform  or  variable  intensity, 
it  is  mixed  up,  in  the  general  methods  of  excitement,  with  certain  noises,  which 
express  greater  or  less  irregularities  in  the  motion  of  the  air.  In  wind  instruments 
where  the  tones  are  maintained  by  a  stream  of  air,  we  generally  hear  more  or  less  ^ 
whizzing  and  hissing  of  the  air  which  breaks  against  the  sharp  edges  of  the 
mouthpiece.    In  strings,  rods,  or  plates  excited  by  a  violin  bow,  we  usually  hear 
a  good  deal  of  noise  from  the  rubbing.    The  hairs  of  the  bow  are  naturally  full  of 
many  minnte  irregularities,  the  resinous  coating  is  not  spread  over  it  with  absolute 
evenness,  and  there  are  also  little  inequalities  in  the  motion  of  the  arm  which 
holds  the  bow  and  in  the  amount  of  pressure,  all  of  which  influence  the  motion 
of  the  string,  and  make  the  tone  gf  a  bad  instrument  or  an  unskilful  performer 
rough,  scraping,  and  variable.    We  shall  not  be  able  to  explain  the  nature  of  the 
motions  of  the  air  and  sensations  of  the  ear  which  correspond  to  these  noises  till 
we  have  investigated  the  conception  of  heats.    Those  who  listen  to  music  make 
themselves  deaf  to  these  noises  by  purposely  withdrawing  attention  from  them,  but 
a  slight  amoimt  of  attention  generally  makes  them  very  evident  for  all  tones  pro- 
duced by  blowing  or  rubbing.     It  is  well  known  that  most  consonants  in  human 
speech  are  characterised  by  the  maintenance  of  similar  noises,  as  F,  V ;  S,  Z  ;  TH  f 
in  thin  and  in  then ;  the  Scotch  and  German  guttural  CH,  and  Dutch  G.    For 
some  the  tone  is  made  still  more  irregular  by  trilling  parts  of  the  mouth,  as  for 
B  and  L.     In  the  case  of  B  the  stream  of  air  is  periodically  entirely  interrupted  by 
trilling  the  uvula  *  or  the  tip  of  the  tongue  ;  and  we  thus  obtain  an  intermitting 
sound  to  which  these  interruptions  give  a  peculiar  jarring  character.    In  the  case 
of  L  the  soft  side  edges  of  the  tongue  are  moved  by  the  stream  of  air,  and,  without 
completely  interrupting  the  tone,  produce  inequalities  in  its  strength. 

Even  the  vowels  themselves  are  not  free  from  such  noises,  although  they  are 
kept  more  in  the  background  by  the  musical  character  of  the  tones  of  the  voice. 
Donders  first  drew  attention  to  tiiese  noises,  which  are  partly  identical  with  those 
which  are  produced  when  the  corresponding  vowels  are  indicated  in  low  voiceless 

*  [In  the  northern  parts  of  Germany  and  of  There  are  also  many  other  trillg,  into  which, 
France,  and  in  Noitiiumberland,  but  not  other-  as  into  other  phonetio  details,  it  is  not  neces- 
wise  in  England,  except  as  an  organic  defect,      sary  to  enter.— Translator.] 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQ IC 


68  CONCEPTION  OP  MUSICAL  QUALITY  AND  TONE,     pabt  i. 

speech.  They  are  strongest  for  ee  m  see,  the  French  u  in  vu  (which  is  nearly  the 
same  as  the  Norfolk  and  Devon  oo  in  too),  and  for  oo  in  too.  For  these  vowels  they 
can  be  made  audible  even  when  speaking  aloud.*  By  simply  increasing  their  force 
the  vowel  ee  in.  see  becomes  the  consonant  y  in  yon,  and  the  vowel  oo  in  too  the 
consonant  w  in  wan,\  For  a  in  a/rt,  a  mat,  em  met,  there,  and  o  in  more,  the 
noises  appear  to  me  to  be  produced  in  the  glottis  alone  when  speaking  gently,  and 
to  be  absorbed  into  the  voice  when  speaking  aloud.j:  It  is  remarkable  that  in 
speaking,  the  vowels  a  in  art,  a  in  at,  and  e  in  met,  there,  are  produced  with  less 
musical  tone  than  in  singing.  It  seems  as  if  a  feeling  of  greater  compression  in 
the  larynx  caused  the  tuneful  tone  of  the  voice  to  give  way  to  one  of  a  more  jarring 
character  which  admits  of  more  evident  articulation.  The  greater  intensity  thus 
given  to  the  noises,  appears  in  this  case  to  facilitate  the  characterisation  of  the 
peculiar  vowel  quality.     In  singing,  on  the  contrary,  we  try  to  favour  the  musical 

IF  part  of  its  quality  and  hence  often  render  the  articulation  somewhat  obscure.§ 

Such  accompanying  noises  and  little  inequalities  in  the  motion  of  the  air, 
furnish  much  that  is  characteristic  in  the  tones  of  musical  instruments,  and  in  the 
vocal  tones  of  speech  which  correspond  to  the  different  positions  of  the  mouth ; 
but  besides  these  there  are  numerous  peculiarities  of  quality  belonging  to  the 
musical  tone  proper,  that  is,  to  the  perfectly  regular  portion  of  the  motion  of  the 
air.  The  importance  of  these  can  be  better  appreciated  by  listening  to  musical 
instruments  or  human  voices,  from  such  a  distance  that  the  comparatively  weaker 
noises  are  no  longer  audible.  Notwithstanding  the  absence  of  these  noises,  it  is 
generally  possible  to  discriminate  the  different  musical  instruments,  although  it 
must  be  acknowledged  that  under  such  circumstances  the  tone  of  a  French  horn 
may  be  occasionally  mistaken  for  that  of  the  singing  voice,  or  a  violoncello  may 
be  confused  with  an  harmonium.  For  the  human  voice,  consonants  first  disappear 
at  a  distance,  because  they  are  characterised  by  noises,  but  M,  N,  and  the  vowels 

f  can  be  distinguished  at  a  greater  distance.  The  formation  of  M  and  N  in  so  far 
resembles  that  of  vowels,  that  no  noise  of  wind  is  generated  in  any  part  of  the 
cavity  of  the  mouth,  which  is  perfectly  closed,  and  the  sound  of  the  voice  escapes 
through  the  nose.  The  mouth  merely  forms  a  resonance  chamber  which  alters  the 
quality  of  tone.  It  is  interesting  in  calm  weather  to  listen  to  the  voices  of  men 
who  are  descending  from  high  hills  to  the  plain.  Words  can  no  longer  be  recog- 
nised, or  at  most  only  such  as  are  composed  of  M,  N,  and  vowels,  as  Mamma,  No, 
Noon,  But  the  vowels  contained  in  the  spoken  words  are  easily  distinguished. 
Wanting  the  thread  which  connects  them  into  words  and  sentences,  they  form  a 
strange  series  of  alternations  of  quality  and  singular  inflections  of  tone. 

In  the  present  chapter  we  shall  at  first  disregard  all  irregular  portions  of  the 
motion  of  the  air,  and  the  mode  in  which  sounds  commence  or  terminate,  directing 
our  attention  solely  to  the  musical  part  of  the  tone,  properly  so  called,  which 
corresponds  to  a  uniformly  sustained  and  regularly  periodic  motion  of  the  air, 

f  and  we  shall  endeavour  to  discover  the  relations  between  Ijie  quality  of  the  sound 

*  [At  the  Com6die  Francaise  I  have  heard  the  important  phonetio  observations  In   the 

M.  Gk>t  pronounce  the  word  oui  and  Mme.  iezi,— Translator.] 

Provost-Ponsin  pronounce  the  last  syllable  of  §   [These  observations  must  not  be  con- 

liachis  entirely  without  voice  tones,  and  yet  sidered  as  exhausting  the  subject  of  the  dif- 

make  them  audible  throughout  the  theatre. —  ference  between  the  singing  and  the  speak- 

TranslatorJ]  ing  voice,  which  requires  a  peculiar   stadj 

f  [That  this  is  not  the  whole  of  the  pheno-  here  merely  indicated.    See  my  PronundattKm 

menon  is  shewn  by  the  words  ye^  woo.    The  for  Sinaers  (Curwen)  and  Speech   in   Sena 

whole  subject  is  discussed  at  length  in  my  (Novello).  The  difference  between  English  aji^ 

Early  English  Pronunciation,  pp.  1092- 1094,  German  habits  of  speaking  and  singing  must 

and  1149-11  $1,— Translator.]  also  be  borne  in  mind,  and  allowed  for   by 

X  [By  *  speaking  gently  *  (leise)  seems  to  the  reader.    The  English  vowels  given  in  the 

be  meant  either  speaking  absolutely  without  text  are  not  the  perfect  equivalents  of  ProL 

voice,  that  is  with  an  open  glottis,  or  in  a  Helmholtz's    German    sounds.      The    noises 

whisper,  with  the  glottis  nearly  closed.    For  which  accompany  the  vowels  are  not   nearly 

voice  the  glottis  is  quite  closed,  and  this  is  so  marked  in  English  as  in  German,  but  they 

indicated  by  *  speaking  aloud  *  {b&itn  la/ulen  differ  very  mudi  locally,  even  in  England.— 

Spre^ien).    It  would  lead  too  far  to  discuss  Translator.] 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


CHAP.  V.  I.  TONES  WITH  NO  UPPEE  PARTIALS.  69 

and  lis  composition  out  of  individual  simple  tones.    The  peculiarities  of  quality 
of  sound  belonging  to  this  division,  we  shall  briefly  call  its  musical  quality. 

The  object  of  the  present  chapter  is,  therefore,  to  describe  the  different  com- 
position of  musical  tones  as  produced  by  different  instruments,  for  the  purpose  of 
shewing  how  different  modes  of  combining  the  upper  partial  tones  correspond  to 
characteristic  varieties  of  musical  quality.  Certain  general  rules  will  result  for 
the  arrangement  of  the  upper  partials  which  answer  to  such  species  of  musical 
quality  as  are  called,  soft^  pisrcingt  braying^  hollow  or  poor^  full  or  rich,  dull, 
bright,  crisp,  pungent,  and  so  on.  Independently  of  our  inmiediate  object  (the 
determination  of  the  physiological  action  of  the  ear  in  the  discrimination  of 
musical  quality,  which  is  reserved  for  the  following  chapter),  the  results  of  this 
investigation  are  important  for  the  resolution  of  purely  musical  questions  in  later 
chapters,  because  they  shew  us  how  rich  in  upper  partials,  good  musical  qualities 
of  tone  are  found  to  be,  and  also  pomt  out  the  peculiarities  of  musical  quality  5 
fftvoured  on  those  musical  instruments,  for  which  the  quality  of  tone  has  been  to 
some  extent  abandoned  to  the  caprice  of  the  maker. 

Since  physicists  have  worked  comparatively  little  at  this  subject  I  shall  be 
forced  to  enter  somewhat  more  minutely  into  the  mechanism  by  which  the  tones 
of  several  instruments  are  produced,  than  will  be,  perhaps,  agreeable  to  many  of 
my  readers.  For  such  the  principal  results  collected  at  the  end  of  this  chapter  will 
suffice.  On  the  other  hand,  I  must  ask  indulgence  for  leaving  many  large  gaps 
in  this  almost  unexplored  region,  and  for  confining  myself  principally  to  instru- 
ments sufficiently  well  known  for  us  to  obtain  a  tolerably  satisfactory  view  of  the 
source  of  their  tones.  In  this  inquiry  lie  rich  materials  for  interesting  acoustical 
work.  Bat  I  have  felt  bound  to  confine  myself  to  what  was  necessary  for  the 
continuation  of  the  present  investigation. 

I .  Musical  Tones  without  Upper  Partials.  ^ 

We  begin  with  such  musical  tones  as  are  not  decomposable,  but  consist  of  a 
single  simple  tone.  These  are  most  readily  and  purely  produced  by  holding  a 
struck  tuning-fork  over  the  mouth  of  a  resonance  tube,  as  has  been  described  in 
the  last  chapter  (p.  54^).*  These  tones  are  uncommonly  soft  and  free  from  all 
shrillness  and  roughness.  As  already  remarked,  they  appear  to  he  comparatively 
deep,  so  that  such  as  correspond  to  the  deep  tones  of  a  bass  voice  produce  the 
impression  of  a  most  remarkable  and  unusual  depth.  The  musical  quahty  of  such 
deep  simple  tones  is  also  rather  dull.  The  simple  tones  of  the  soprano  pitch 
sound  bright,  but  even  those  corresponding  to  the  highest  tones  of  a  soprano  voice 
are  very  soft,  without  a  trace  of  that  cutting,  rasping  shrillness  which  is  displayed 
by  most  instruments  at  such  pitches,  with  ihe  exception,  perhaps,  of  the  flute,  for 
which  the  tones  are  very  nearly  simple,  being  accompanied  with  very  few  and 
faint  upper  partials.  Among  vowels,  the  00  in  too  comes  nearest  to  a  simple  tone, 
but  even  this  vowel  is  not  entirely  free  from  upper  partials.  On  comparing  the  il 
musical  quahty  of  a  simple  tone  thus  produced  with  that  of  a  compound  tone  in 
which  the  first  harmonic  upper  partial  tones  are  developed,  the  latter  will  be  found 
to  be  more  tuneful,  metallic,  and  brilliant.  Even  the  vowel  00  in  too,  altliough 
the  dullest  and  least  tuneful  of  all  vowels,  is  sensibly  more  brilHant  and  less  dull 
than  a  simple  tone  of  the  same  pitch.  The  series  of  the  first  six  partials  of  a 
compound  tone  may  be  regarded  musically  as  a  major  chord  with  a  very  predominant 
fundamental  tone,  and  in  fact  the  musical  quality  of  a  compound  tone  possessing 
these  partials,  as,  for  example,  a  fine  singing  voice,  when  heard  beside  a  simple  tone, 
very  distinctly  produces  the  agreeable  effect  of  a  consonant  chord. 

Since  the  form  of  simple  waves  of  known  periodic  time  is  completely  given 
when  their  amphtude  is  given,  simple  tones  of  the  same  pitch  can  only  differ 
in  force  and  not  in  musical  quality.    In  fact,  the  difference  of  quahty  remains 

*  On  possible  sources  of  disturbance,  see  Appendix  IV. 

Digitized  by  VjOOQIC 


70  TONES  WITH  INHARMONIC  UPPER  PARTIALS.        pabt  i. 

perfectly  indistinguishable,  whether  the  simple  tone  is  conducted  to  the  external 
air  in  the  preceding  methods  by  a  tmiing-fork  and  a  resonance  tube  of  any  given 
material,  glass,  metal,  or  pasteboard,  or  by  a  string,  provided  only  that  we  guard 
against  any  chattering  in  the  apparatus. 

Simple  tones  accompanied  only  by  the  noise  of  rushing  wind  can  also  be  pro- 
duced, as  already  mentioned,  by  blowing  over  the  mouth  of  bottles  with  necks 
(p.  6oc).  If  we  disregard  the  friction  of  the  air,  the  proper  musical  quality  of  such 
tones  is  really  the  same  as  that  produced  by  tuning-forks. 

2.  Musical  Tones  with  Inharmonic  Upper  Partials. 

Nearest  to  musical  tones  without  any  upper  partials  are  those  with  secondary 
tones  which  are  inharmonic  to  the  prime,  and  such  tones,  therefore,  in  strictness, 

^  should  not  be  reckoned  as  musical  tones  at  all.  They  are  exceptionally  used  in 
artistic  music,  but  only  when  it  is  contrived  that  the  prime  tone  should  be  so  much 
more  powerful  than  the  secondary  tones,  that  the  existence  of  the  latter  may  be 
ignored.  Hence  they  are  placed  here  next  to  the  simple  tones,  because  musically 
they  are  available  only  for  the  more  or  less  good  simple  tones  which  they  represent. 
The  first  of  these  are  tuning-forks  themselves,  when  they  are  struck  and  applied 
to  a  sounding  board,  or  brought  very  near  the  ear.  The  [inharmonic]  upper  partials 
of  tuning-forks  lie  very  high.  In  those  which  I  have  examined,  the  first  made 
from  5*8  to  6*6  as  many  vibrations  in  the  same  time  as  the  prime  tone,  and  hence 
lay  between  its  third  diminished  Fifth  and  major  Sixth.  The  pitch  numbers  of 
these  high  upper  partial  tones  were  to  one  another  as  the  squares  of  the  odd 
numbers.  In  the  time  that  the  first  upper  partial  would  execute  3  x  3=9  vibra- 
tions, the  next  would  execute  5  x  5=25*  and  the  next  7  x  7=^49,  and  so  on.  Their 
pitch,  therefore,  increases  with  extraordinary  rapidity,  and  tiiey  are  usually  all 

^  inharmonic  with  the  prime,  though  some  of  them  may  exceptionally  become 
harmonic.  If  we  call  the  prime  tone  of  the  fork  c,  the  next  succeeding  tones  are 
nearly  a"[>,  dT^  &'^.*  These  high  secondary  tones  produce  a  bright  inharmonic 
clink,  which  is  easily  heard  at  a  considerable  distance  when  the  fork  is  first  struck, 
whereas  when  it  is  brought  close  to  the  ear,  the  prime  tone  alone  is  heard.  The 
ear  readily  separates  the  prime  from  the  upper  tones  and  has  no  inclination  to  fuse 
them.  The  high  simple  tones  usually  die  off  rapidly,  while  the  prime  tone  remains 
audible  for  a  long  time.  It  should  be  remarked,  however,  that  the  mutual  relations 
of  the  proper  tones  of  tuning-forks  differ  somewhat  according  to  the  form  of  the 
fork,  and  hence  the  above  indications  must  be  looked  upon  as  merely  approximate. 
In  theoretical  determinations  of  the  upper  partial  tones,  each  prong  of  the  fork 
may  be  regarded  as  a  rod  fixed  at  one  end. 

The  same  relations  hold  for  straight  elastic  rods,  which,  as  already  mentioned, 
when  struck,  give  rather  high  inharmonic  upper  partial  tones.    When  such  a  rod 

f  is  firmly  supported  at  the  two  nodal  lines  of  its  prime  tone,  the  continuance  of 
that  tone  is  favoured  in  preference  to  the  other  higher  tones,  and  hence  the  latter 
disturb  the  effect  very  slightly,  more  especially  as  they  rapidly  die  away  after  the 
rod  has  been  struck.    Such  rods,  however,  are  not  suitable  for  real  artistic  music, 

*  [On  oaloolating  the  number  of  cents  (as  henoe  it  is  called  dT*  in  the  text.    The  interval 

in  App.  XX.  sect.  C.),  we  find  that  the  first  to  the  next  tone  is  25  :  49  or   1165  cents, 

tone  mentioned,  which  vibrates  from  5*8  to  Adding  this  to  the  former  numbers  the  interval 

6*6  as  fait  as  the  prime,  makes  an  interval  with  the  prime  must  be  between  5977  and 

with  it  of  from  3043  to  3267  ot.,  so  that  if  6201  cents,  or  between  b^  +  77  and  d"-^,  for 

the  prime  is  called  c,  the  note  lies  between  which  in  the  text  c^U  ia  selected.    The  inde- 

^'bi-43,  and  af'—^Sf  where  g"b  and  a"  are  terminacy  arises  from  the  difficulty  of  finding 

the  third  diminished  Fifth  and  major  Sixth  of  the  pitch  of  the  first  inharmonic  upper  partiaL 

the  prime  c  mentioned  in  the  text.    This  Prof.  The  intervals  between  that  and  the  next  upper 

Helmholtz  calls  a'"bt  or  3200  cents.    Then  the  partials  are  9  :  25  or  1769  ct.,  9  :  49  or  2934 

interval  between  this  partial  and  the  next  is  ct.,  9  :  81  or  3699  ct.,  and  so  on.    The  word 

9  :  25  or   1769  ct.,  and   hence   the  interval  'inhannonic*  has  been  inserted-  in  the  text, 

with  the  prime  is  between  4812  and    5036  as  tuning-forks  have  also  generally  harmonic 

cents,  or  lies  between  0^^+12  and  d'^  +  36,  and  upper  purtials.  See  p.  54^^,  note.— Translator.] 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


CHAP.  V.  2.    TONES  WITH  INHARMONIC  UPPER  PARTIALS.  71 

although  they  have  lately  been  introduced  for  military  and  dance  music  on  account 
of  their  penetrating  qualities  of  tone.  Glass  rods  or  plates,  and  wooden  rods,  were 
formerly  used  in  this  way  for  the  glass  harmonicon  and  the  straw-fiddle  or  wood 
harmonicon.  The  rods  were  inserted  between  two  pairs  of  intertwisted  strings, 
which  grasped  them  at  their  two  nodal  lines.  The  wooden  rods  in  the  German 
straw-fiddle  were  simply  laid  on  straw  cylinders.  They  were  struck  with  hammers 
of  wood  or  cork. 

The  only  effect  of  the  material  of  the  rods  on  the  quality  of  tone  in  these 
cases,  consists  in  the  greater  or  less  length  of  time  that  it  allows  the  proper  tones 
at  different  pitches  to  continue.  These  secondary  tones,  including  the  higher  ones, 
usually  continue  to  sound  longest  in  elastic  metal  of  fine  uniform  consistency, 
because  its  greater  mass  giyes  it  a  greater  tendency  to  continue  in  any  state  of 
motion  which  it  has  once  assumed,  and  among  metals  the  most  perfect  elasticity 
is  found  in  steel,  and  the  better  alloys  of  copper  and  zinc,  or  copper  and  tin.  In  ^ 
slightly  alloyed  precious  metals,  their  greater  specific  gravity  lengthens  the  dura- 
tion ,of  the  tone,  notwithstanding  their  inferior  elasticity.  Superior  elasticity 
appears  to  favour  the  continuance  of  the  higher  proper  tones,  because  imperfect 
elasticity  and  friction  generally  seem  to  damp  rapid  more  quickly  than  slow  vibra-  ^ 
tions.  Hence  I  think  that  I  may  describe  the  general  characteristic  of  what  is 
usually  called  a  metallic  quality  of  tone,  as  the  comparatively  continuous  and 
uniform  maintenance  of  higher  upper  partial  tones.  The  quality  of  tone  for  glass 
is  similar ;  but  as  it  breaks  when  violently  agitated,  the  tone  is  always  weak  and 

FlO.  31. 


soft,  and  it  is  also  comparatively  high,  and  dies  rapidly  away,  on  account  of  the 
smaller  mass  of  the  vibrating  body.  In  wood  the  mass  is  small,  the  internal 
structure  comparatively  rough,  being  full  of  countless  interstices,  and  the  elasticity 
also  comparatively  imperfect,  so  that  the  proper  tones,  especially  the  higher  ones, 
rapidly  die  away.  And  for  this  reason  the  straw-fiddle  or  wood  harmonicon  is  per- 
haps more  satisfactory  to  a  musical  ear,  than  harmonicons  formed  of  steel  or  glass 
rods  or  plates,  with  their  piercing  inharmonic  upper  partial  tones, — at  least  so  &r 
as  simple  tones  are  suitable  for  music  at  all,  of  which  I  shall  have  to  speak  later  on.* 

For  all  of  these  instruments  which  have  to  be  struck,  the  hammers  are  made 
of  wood  or  cork,  and  covered  with  leather.  This  renders  the  highest  upper 
partials  much  weaker  than  if  only  hard  metal  hammers  were  employed.  Greater  ^ 
hardness  of  the  striking  mass  produces  greater  discontinuities  in  the  original 
motion  of  the  plate.  The  influence  exerted  by  the  manner  of  striking  will  be 
considered  more  in  detail,  in  reference  to  strings,  where  it  is  also  of  much  impor- 
tance. 

According  to  Ghladni's  discoveries,  elastic  plates ,  cut  in  circular,  oval,  square, 
oblong,  triangular,  or  hexagonal  forms,  will  sound  in  a  great  number  of  different 
vibrational  forms,  usually  producing  simple  tones  which  are  mutually  inharmonic. 
Fig.  21  gives  the  more  simple  vibrational  forms  of  a  circular  plate.  Much  more 
complicated  forms  occur  when  several  circles  or  additional  diameters  appear  as 
nodal  lines,  or  where  both  circles  and  diameters  occur.  Supposing  the  vibrational 
form  A  to  give  the  tone  c,  the  others  give  the  following  proper  tones : — 

*  [In  Java  the  principal  music  is  produced  the  rods  are  laid  on  the  edges  of  boat-shaped 
by  harmonicons  of  metal  or  wooden  rods  and  vessels,  like  old  fashion  cheese-trays,  and  kept 
kettle-shaped  gongs.  The  wooden  harmonicons  in  position  by  nails  passing  loosely  through 
are  frequent  8d(so  in  Asia  and  Africa.    In  Java      holes.    See  App.  XX.  sect.  K,— Translator,]    j 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQ IC 


72 


TONES  WITH  INHARMONIC  UPPER  PARTIALS. 


PAKF  I. 


Number 

of  Nodal 

Circles 

Namber  of  Diameters 

o 

I 

2          1          3 

4 

5 

O 

I 
2 

9"»  + 

b'b 

C 

d* 

c" 

i^'-y"« 

This  shews  that  many  proper  tones  of  nearly  the  same  pitch  are  produced  by  a 
plate  of  this  kind.  When  a  plate  is  struck,  those  proper  tones  which  have  no 
node  at  the  point  struck,  will  all  sound  together.  To  obtain  a  particular  deter- 
minate tone  it  is  of  advantage  to  support  the  plate  in  points  which  lie  in  the  nodal 
lines  of  that  tone ;  because  those  proper  tones  which  have  no  node  in  those  poinds 
will  then  die  off  more  rapidly.    For  example,  if  a  circular  plate  is  supported  at 

^  3  points  in  the  nodal  circle  of  fig.  2 1 ,  G  (p.  71c),  and  is  struck  exactly  in  its  middle, 
the  simple  tone  called  ^  in  the  table,  which  belongs  to  that  form,  will  be  heard, 
and  all  those  other  proper  tones  which  have  diameters  as  some  of  their  nodal 
lines  *  will  be  very  weak,  for  example  c,  d\  c"^  g'\  h^  in  the  table.  In  the  same 
way  the  tone  g'%  with  two  nodal  circles,  dies  off  inmiediately,  because  the  points 
of  support  fall  on  one  of  its  ventral  segments,  and  the  first  proper  tone  which  can 
sound  loudly  at  the  same  time  is  that  corresponding  to  three  nodal  circles,  one  of 
its  nodal  lines  being  near  to  that  of  No.  2.  But  this  is  3  Octaves  and  more  than 
a  whole  Tone  higher  than  the  proper  tone  of  No.  2,  and  on  account  of  this  great 
interval  does  not  disturb  ilie  latter.  Hence  a  disc  thus  struck  gives  a  tolerably 
good  musical  tone,  whereas  plates  in  general  produce  sounds  composed  of  many  in- 
harmonic proper  tones  of  nearly  the  same  pitch,  giving  an  einpty  tin-kettle  sort  of 
qualityi  which  cannot  be  used  in  music.  But  even  when  the  disc  is  properly  sup- 
ported the  tone  dies  away  rapidly,  at  least  in  the  case  of  glass  plates,  because 

\  contact  at  many  points,  even  when  nodal,  sensibly  impedes  the  freedom  of  vibra- 
tion. 

The  sound  of  helli  is  also  accompanied  by  inharmonic  secondary  tones,  which, 
however,  do  not  lie  so  close  to  one  another  as  those  of  flat  plates.  The  vibrations 
which  usually  arise  have  4,  6,  8,  10,  &c.,  nodal  lines  extending  from  the  vertex  of 
the  bell  to  its  margin,  at  equal  intervals  from  each  other.  The  corresponding 
proper  tones  for  glass  bells  which  have  approximatively  the  same  thickness 
throughout,  are  nearly  as  the  squares  of  the  numbers  2,  3,  4,  5,  so  that  if  we  call 
the  lowest  tone  c,  we  have  for  the 


Nnmber  of  nodal  lines  . 

4 

6 

8 

10 

12 

Tones  ../... 
Cento    .        .                ... 

c 
0 

1404 

2400 

ft- 
3173 

3804 

The  tones,  hoT/aver,  vary  with  the  greater  or  less  thickness  of  the  wall  of  the 
H  bell  towards  the  margin,  and  it  appears  to  be  an  essential  point  in  the  art  of 
casting  bells,  tr  make  the  deeper  proper  tones  mutually  harmonic  by  giving  the 
bell  a  certaiTi  empirical  form.  According  to  the  observations  of  the  organist 
01eitz,t  the  bell  cast  for  the  cathedral  at  Erfurt  in  1477  has  the  following  proper 
tones:  E,  €,  ^  h^  e\  g%,  h',  c"#.  The  [former]  bell  of  St.  Paul's,. London,  gave 
a  and  c%.  Hemony  of  Ziitphen,  a  master  in  the  seventeenth  century,  required  a 
good  bell  to  hava  three  Octaves,  two  Fifths,  one  major  and  one  minor  Third.  The 
deepest  tone  is  not  the  strongest.  The  body  of  the  bell  when  struck  gives  a 
deeper  tone  than  the  '  sound  bow,'  but  the  latter  gives  the  loudest  tone.  Probably 
other  vibrational  forms  of  bells  are  also  possible  in  which  nodal  circles  are  formed 


*  Provided  that  the  supported  points  do 
not  happen  to  belong  to  a  system  of  diameters 
juaking  equal  angles  with  eaoh  other. 

t  'Historical  Notes  on  the  Great  Bell 
And  the  other   Bells   in   Erfurt   Cathedral' 


{Qeschichlliches  fiber  die  grosse  Glocke  und, 
die  ilbrigen  Oloc/cen  des  Domes  eu  Erfurt), 
Erfurt,  1 867. -See  also  Sohafhautl  in  the 
Kunst'  und  GewerbeblaU  fUr  das  KOnigreich 
Bayem,  1868,  liv.  325  to  350;  385  to  427. 
Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


CHAP.  V.  2.    TONES  WITH  INHARMONIC  UPPER  PARTIALS. 


73; 


parallel  to  the  margin.  But  these  seem  to  be  produced  with  difficulty  and  have 
not  yet  been  examined. 

If  a  bell  is  not  perfectly  symmetrical  in  respect  to  its  axis,  if,  for  example,  the 
wall  is  a  little  thicker  at  one  point  of  its  circumference  than  at  another,  it  will 
give,  on  being  struck,  two  different  tones  of  very  nearly  the  same  pitch,  which  will 
'  beat  *  together.  Four  points  on  the  margin  will  be  found,  separated  from  each 
other  by  quarter-circles,  in  which  only  one  of  these  tones  can  be  heard  without 
accompanying  beats,  and  four  others,  half-way  between  the  pairs  of  the  others, 
where  the  second  tone  only  sounds.  If  the  bell  is  struck  elsewhere  both  tones  are 
heard,  producing  beats,  and  such  beats  may  be  perceived  in  most  bells  as  their 
tone  dies  gradually  away. 

Stretched  membranes  have  also  inharmonic  proper  tones  of  nearly  the  same 
pitch.  For  a  circular  membrane,  of  which  the  deepest  tone  is  c,  these  are,  in  a 
vacuum  and  arranged  in  order  of  pitch,  as  follows : —  f 


Nmnber  of  Nodal  Lines 

Tone 

Diameters 

Circles 

0 

I 
2 

o 

I 

o 

0 
O 
0 

I 
I 

2 

C 

ab 

d%  +0-1  • 

d'    +0-2 
^    —  0'2 

6'b  +  o-i 

These  tones  rapidly  die  out.  If  the  membranes  sound  in  air,t  or  are  associated 
with  an  air  chamber,  as  in  the  kettledrum,  the  relation  of  the  proper  tones  may 
be  altered.  No  detailed  investigations  have  yet  been  made  on  the  secondary  tones 
of  the  kettledrum.  The  kettledrum  is  used  in  artistic  music,  but  only  to  mark  «- 
certain  accents.  It  is  tuned,  indeed,  but  only  to  prevent  ii^jury  to  the  harmony, 
not  for  the  purpose  of  filling  up  chords. 

The  common  character  of  the  instruments  hitherto  described  is,  that,  when 
struck  they  produce  inharmonic  upper  partial  tones.  If  these  are  of  nearly  the 
same  pitch  as  the  prime  tone,  their  quaUty  of  sound  is  in  tbe  highest  degree  un- 
musical, bad,  and  tinkettly.  If  the  secondary  tones  are  of  very  different  pitch 
from  the  prime,  and  weak  in  force,  the  quality  of  sound  is  more  musical,  as  for 
example  in  tuning-forks,  harmonicons  of  rods,  and  bells ;  and  such  tones  are  applic- 
able for  marches  and  other  boisterous  music,  principally  intended  to  mark  time. 
But  for  really  artistic  music,  such  instruments  as  these  have  always  been  rejected, 
as  they  ought  to  be,  for  the  inharmonic  secondary  tones,  although  they  rapidly  die 
away,  always  disturb  the  harmony  most  unpleasantly,  renewed  as  they  are  at  every 
fresh  blow.  A  very  striking  example  of  this  was  furnished  by  a  company  of  bell- 
rmgers,  said  to  be  Scotch,  that  lately  travelled  about  Germany,  and  performed  all  «- 
kinds  of  musical  pieces,  some  of  which  had  an  artistic  character.  The  accuracy 
and  skill  of  the  performance  was  undeniable,  but  the  musical  effect  was  detestable, 
on  account  of  the  heap  of  false  secondary  tones  which  accompanied  the  music, 
although  care  was  taken  to  damp  each  bell  as  soon  as  the  proper  duration  of  its 
note  had  expired,  by  placing  it  on  a  table  covered  with  cloth. 

Sonorous  bodies  with  inharmonic  partials,  may  be  also  set  in  action  by  violin 
bows,  and  then  by  properly  damping  them  in  a  nodal  line  of  the  desired  tone,  the 
secondary  tones  which  lie  near  it  can  be  prevented  from  interfering.  One  simple 
tone  then  predominates  distinctly,  and  it  might  consequently  be  used  for  musical 
purposes.  But  when  the  violin  bow  is  applied  to  any  bodies  with  inharmonic 
upper  partial  tones,  as  tuning-forks,  plates,  bells,  we  hear  a  strong  scratching 


*  [These  decimals  represent  tenths  of  a 
tone,  or  20  cents  for  the  first  place.  As  there 
can  be  no  sounds  in  a  vacuum,  these  notes 
are   merely  used  to  conveniently  symbolise 


numbers  of  vibrations  in  a  second.— Trar^s- 
lator.] 

t  See  /.  Bourgelt  L'Institut,  xxzviii.,  1870, 
pp.  189,  19a 

Digitized  by  VjOOQlC 


74  MUSICAL  TONES  OF  STRINGS.  paet  i. 

sound,  which  on  investigation  with  resonators,  is  found  to  consist  mainly  of  these 
same  inharmonic  secondary  tones  of  such  bodies,  not  sounding  continuously  but 
only  in  short  irregular  fits  and  starts.  Intermittent  tones,  as  I  have  already  noted, 
produce  the  effect  of  grating  or  scratching.  It  is  only  when  the  body  excited  by 
the  violin  bow  has  harmonic  upper  partials,  that  it  can  perfectly  accommodate  itse^ 
to  every  impulse  of  the  bow,  and  give  a  really  musical  quality  of  tone.  The 
reason  of  this  is  that  any  required  periodic  motion  such  as  the  bow  aims  at  pro- 
ducing., can  be  compounded  of  motions  corresponding  to  harmonic  upper  partial 
tones,  but  not  of  other,  inharmonic  vibrations. 

3.  M^8^cal  Tones  of  Strings. 

We  now  proceed  to  the  analysis  of  musical  tones  proper,  which  are  characterised 
H  by  harmonic  upper  partials.  These  may  be  best  classified  according  to  their  mode 
of  excitement :  i.  By  striking.  2.  By  bowing.  3.  By  blowing  against  a  sharp 
edge.  4.  By  blowing  against  elastic  tongues  or  vibrators.  The  two  first  classes 
comprehend  stringed  instruments  alone,  as  longitudinally  vibrating  rods,  the  only 
other  instruments  producing  harmonic  upper  partial  tones,  are  not  used  for  musical 
purposes.  The  third  class  embraces  flutes  and  the  flute  or  flue  pipes  of  organs ; 
the  fourth  all  other  wind  instruments,  including  the  human  voice. 

Strings  excited  by  Striking. — Among  musical  instruments  at  present  in  use, 
this  section  embraces  the  pianoforte,  harp,  guitar,  and  zither ;  among  physical, 
the  monochord,  arranged  for  an  accurate  examination  of  the  laws  controlling  the 
vibrations  of  strings ;  the  pizzicato  of  bowed  instruments  must  also  be  placed  in 
this  category.  We  have  idready  mentioned  that  the  musical  tones  produced  by 
strings  whidi  are  struck  or  plucked,  contain  numerous  upper  partial  tones.  We 
have  the  advantage  of  possessing  a  complete  theory  for  the  motion  of  plucked 
%  strings,  by  which  the  force  of  their  upper  partial  tones  may  be  determined.  In 
the  last  chapter  we  compared  some  of  the  conclusions  of  this  theory  with  the 
results  of  experiment,  and  found  them  agree.  A  similarly  complete  theory  maybe 
formed  for  the  case  of  a  string  which  has  been  struck  in  one  of  its  points  by  a 
hard  sharp  edge.  The  problem  is  not  so  simple  when  soft  elastic  hammers  are 
used,  such  as  those  of  the  pianoforte,  but  even  in  this  case  it  is  possible  to  assign 
a  theory  for  the  motion  of  the  string  which  embraces  at  least  the  most  essential 
features  of  the  process,  and  indicates  the  force  of  the  upper  partial  tones.* 

The  force  of  the  upper  partial  tones  in  a  struck  string,  depends  in  general 
on: — 

1.  The  nature  of  the  stroke. 

2.  The  place  struck. 

3.  The  density,  rigidity,  and  elasticity  of  the  string. 

First,  as  to  the  nature  of  the  stroke.  The  string  may  be  plucked,  by  drawing 
%  it  on  one  side  with  the  finger  or  a  point  (the  plectrum,  or  the  ring  of  the  zither- 
player),  and  then  letting  it  go.  This  is  a  usual  mode  of  exciting  a  string  in  a  great 
number  of  ancient  and  modem  stringed  instruments.  Among  the  modem,  I  need 
only  mention  the  harp,  guitar,  and  zither.  Or  else  the  string  may  be  struck  with 
a  hammer-shaped  body,  as  in  the  pianoforte.f  I  have  already  remarked  that  the 
strength  and  number  of  the  upper  partial  tones  increases  with  the  number  and 
abruptness  of  the  discontinuities  in  the  motion  excited.  This  fact  determines  the 
various  modes  of  exciting  a  string.  When  a  string  is  plucked,  the  finger,  before 
quitting  it,  removes  it  from  its  position  of  rest  throughout  its  whole  length.  A 
discontinuity  in  the  string  arises  only  by  its  forming  a  more  or  less  acute  angle  at 
the  place  where  it  wraps  itself  about  the  finger  or  point.  The  angle  is  more  acute 
for  a  sharp  point  than  for  the  finger.  Hence  the  sharp  point  produces  a  shriUer 
tone  with  a  greater  number  of  high  tinkling  upper  partials,  than  the  finger.    But 

*  See  Appendix  V.  be  struck  by  a  hammer-shaped  body.     See 

t   [I  have  here  omitted  a  few  words  in      pp.  77c  and  jSd'.^Trafislator.] 
which,  by  an  oversight,  the  spinet  was  said  to  , 

Digitized  by  VjOOQ  IC 


CHAP.  V.  3.  MUSICAL  TONES  OF  STBINGS.  75 

in  each  ease  the  intensity  of  the  prime  tone  exceeds  that  of  any  upper  partial.  If 
the  string  is  struck  with  a  sharp-edged  metallic  hammer  which  rehounds  instantly, 
only  the  one  single  point  struck  is  directly  set  in  motion.  Immediately  after  the 
blow  the  remainder  of  the  string  is  at  rest.  It  does  not  move  until  a  wave  of  de- 
flection rises,  and  runs  backwards  and  forwards  over  the  string.  This  limitation 
of  the  original  motion  to  a  single  point  produces  the  most  abrupt  discontinuities, 
and  a  corresponding  long  series  of  upper  partial  tones,  having  intensities,*  in  most 
oases  equalling  or  even  surpassing  that  of  the  prime.  When  the  hammer  is  soft 
and  elastic,  the  motion  has  time  to  spread  before  the  hammer  rebounds.  When 
thus  struck  the  point  of  the  string  in  contact  with  such  a  hammer  is  not  set  in 
motion  with  a  jerk,  but  increases  gradually  and  continuously  in  velocity  during  the 
contact.  The  discontinuity  of  the  motion  is  consequently  much  less,  diminishing 
as  the  softness  of  the  hammer  increases,  and  the  force  of  the  higher  upper  partial 
tones  is  correspondingly  decreased.  y^ 

We  can  easily  convince  ourselves  of  the  correctness  of  these  statements  by 
opening  the  lid  of  any  pianoforte,  and,  keeping  one  of  the  digitals  down  with  a 
weight,  so  as  to  free  the  string  from  the  damper,  plucking  the  string  at  pleasure 
with  a  finger  or  a  point,  and  striking  it  with  a  metaJhc  edge  or  the  pianoforte  ham- 
mer itself.  The  qualities  of  tone  thus  obtained  will  be  entirely  different.  When 
the  string  is  struck  or  plucked  with  hard  metal,  the  tone  is  piercing  and  tinkling, 
and  a  little  attention  enables  us  to  hear  a  multitude  of  very  high  partial  tones. 
These  disappear,  and  the  tone  of  the  string  becomes  less  bright,  but  softer,  and 
more  harmonious,  when  we  pluck  the  string  with  the  soft  finger  or  strike  it  with 
the  soft  hammer  of  the  instrument.  We  also  readily  recognise  the  different  loud- 
ness of  the  prime  tone.  When  we  strike  with  metal,  the  prime  tone  is  scarcely 
heard  and  the  quality  of  tone  is  correspondingly  iJpoT,  The  peculiar  quality  of 
tone  conmionly  termed  poverty,  as  opposed  to  richness,  arises  firom  the  upper 
partials  being  comparatively  too  strong  for  the  prime  tone.  The  prime  tone  is  f 
heard  best  when  the  string  is  plucked  with  a  soft  finger,  which  produces  a  rich  and 
yet  harmonious  quality  of  tone.  The  prime  tone  is  not  so  strong,  at  least  in  the 
middle  and  deeper  octaves  of  the  instrument,  when  the  strings  are  struck  with  the 
pianoforte  hammer,  as  when  they  are  plucked  with  the  finger. 

This  is  the  reason  why  it  has  been  found  advantageous  to  cover  pianoforte  ham- 
mers with  thick  layers  of  felt,  rendered  elastic  by  much  compression.  The  outer 
layers  are  the  softest  and  most  yielding,  the  lower  are  firmer.  The  sur&ce  of  the 
hanuner  comes  in  contact  with  the  string  without  any  audible  impact ;  the  lower 
layers  give  the  elasticity  which  throws  the  hammer  back  from  the  string.  If  you 
remove  a  pianoforte  hammer  and  strike  it  strongly  on  a  wooden  table  or  against  a 
wall,  it  rebounds  from  them  like  an  india-rubber  ball.  The  heavier  the  hammer 
and  the  thicker  the  layers  of  felt— as  in  the  hammers  for  the  lower  octaves — the 
longer  must  it  be  before  it  rebounds  from  the  string.  The  hammers  for  the  upper 
octaves  are  lighter  and  have  thinner  layers  of  felt.  Clearly  the  makers  of  these  ^ 
instruments  have  here  been  led  by  practice  to  discover  certain  relations  of  the 
elasticity  of  the  hanuner  to  the  best  tones  of  the  string.  The  make  of  the  hammer 
has  an  immense  influence  on  the  quality  of  tone.  Theory  shews  that  those  upper 
partial  tones  are  especially  favoured  whose  periodic  time  is  nearly  equal  to  twice 

*  When  intensity  is  here  mentioned,  it  is  .as  the   pitoh  number.     Messrs.  Preeoe  and 

always  measured  objeetiyely,  by  the  via  viva,  Stroh,  Proc.  R,  S,,  vol.  xxviii.  p.  366,  think 

or  mechanical  equivalent  of  work  of  the  eorre-  that  *  loudness  does  not  depend  upon  amplitude 

sponding  motion.    [Mr.  Bosanquet  {Academy,  of  vibration  only,  but  upon  the  quantity  of  air 

Dee.  4,  1875,  p.  580,  col.  i)  points  out  that  put  in  vibration;  and,  therefore,  there  exists 

p.  lod,  note,  and  Chap.  IX.,  paragraph  3,  shew  an  absolute  physical  magnitude  in  acoustics 

this  measure  to  be  inadmissible,  and  adds :  analogous  to  that  of  quantity  of  electricity  or 

'if   we  admit    that  in   similar  organ  pipes  quantity  of  heat,  and  which  may  be  called 

similar  proportions  of  the  wind  supplied  are  quantity  of  sound,*  and  they  illustrate  this  by 

employed  in  the  production  of  tone,  the  me-  the  effect  of  differently  sized  discs  in  their 

chanical  energy  of  notes  of  given  intensity  automatic  phonograph  there  described.     See 

varies  inversely  as  the  vibration  number,*  i.e.  also  App.  XX.  sect.  M.  No.  2,— Translator.'} 


Digitized  by  V^jOOQ IC 


76  MUSICAL  TONES  OP  STRINGS.  pabt  i. 

the  time  during  which  the  hammer  lies  on  the  string,  and  that,  on  the  other  hand, 
those  disappear  whose  periodic  time  is  6,  lo,  14,  &c.,  times  as  great.* 

It  will  generally  be  advantageous,  especially  for  the  deeper  tones,  to  eliminate 
from  the  series  of  upper  partials,  those  which  lie  too  close  to  each  other  to  give  a 
good  compound  tone,  that  is,  from  about  the  seventh  or  eighth  onwards.  Those 
with  higher  ordinal  numbers  are  generally  relatively  weak  of  themselves.  On  ex- 
amining a  new  grand  pianoforte  by  Messrs.  Steinway  of  New  York,  which  was 
remarkable  for  the  evenness  of  its  quality  of  tone,  I  find  that  the  damping  result- 
ing from  the  duration  of  the  stroke  falls,  in  the  deeper  notes,  on  the  ninth  or  tenth 
partials,  whereas  in  the  higher  notes,  the  fourth  and  fifth  partials  were  scarcely  to 
be  got  out  with  the  hammer,  although  they  were  distinctly  audible  when  the  string 
was  plucked  by  the  nail.f  On  the  other  hand  upon  an  older  and  much  used  grand 
piano,  which  originally  sliewed  the  principal  damping  in  the  neighbourhood  of  the 

f  seventh  to  the  fifth  partial  for  middle  and  low  notes,  the  ninth  to  the  thirteenth 
partials  are  now  strongly  developed.  This  is  probably  due  to  a  hardening  of  the 
hammers,  and  certainly  can  only  be  prejudicial  to  the  quality  of  tone.  Observa- 
tions on  these  relations  can  be  easily  made  in  the  method  recommended  on  p.  526,  c. 
Put  the  point  of  the  finger  gently  on  one  of  the  nodes  of  the  tone  of  which  you 
wish  to  discover  the  strength,  and  then  strike  the  string  by  means  of  the  digital. 
By  moving  the  finger  till  the  required  tone  comes  out  most  purely  and  sounds  the 
longest,  the  exact  position  of  the  node  can  be  easily  found.  The  nodes  which  lie 
near  the  striking  point  of  the  hammer,  are  of  course  chiefly  covered  by  the  damper, 
but  the  corresponding  partials  are,  for  a  reason  to  be  given  presently,  relatively 
weak.  Moreover  the  fifth  partial  speaks  well  when  the  string  is  touched  at  two- 
fifths  of  its  length  from  the  end,  and  the  seventh  at  two-sevenths  of  that  length. 
These  positions  are  of  course  quite  free  of  the  damper.  Generally  we  find  all  the 
partials  which  arise  from  the  method  of  striking  used,  when  we  keep  on  striking 

f  while  the  finger  is  gradually  moved  over  the  length  of  the  string.  Touching  the 
shorter  end  of  the  string  between  the  striking  point  and  the  further  bridge  will  thus 
bring  out  the  higher  partials  from  the  ninth  to  the  sixteenth,  which  are  musically 
undesirable. 

The  method  of  calculating  the  strength  of  the  individual  upper  partials,  when 
the  duration  of  the  stroke  of  the  hammer  is  given,  will  be  found  further  on. 

Secondly  as  to  the  pUice  struck.  In  the  last  chapter,  when  verifying  Ohm's 
law  for  the  analysis  of  musical  tones  by  the  ear,  we  remarked  that  whether  strings 
are  plucked  or  struck,  those  upper  partials  disappear  which  have  a  node  at  the 
point  excited.  Conversely ;  those  partials  are  comparatively  strongest  which  have 
a  maximum  displacement  at  that  point.  Generally,  when  the  same  method  of 
striking  is  successively  applied  to  different  points  of  a  string,  the  individual  upper 
partials  increase  or  decrease  with  the  intensity  of  motion,  at  the  point  of  excite- 
ment, for  the  corresponding  simple  vibrations  of  the  string.    The  composition  of 

^  the  musical  tone  of  a  string  can  be  consequently  greatly  varied  by  merely  changing 
the  point  of  excitement. 

Thus  if  a  string  be  struck  in  its  middle,  the  second  partial  tone  disappears, 

*  [The  following  paragraph  on  p.  123  of  several  times.    I  got  out  the  7th  and  9th 

the   iBt  English  edition    has   been   omitted,  harmonic  of    c,  but  on  aocoant  of  difficul- 

and  the  passage  from  *  It  will  generally  be  ties  due  to  the  over-stringing  and  over-barring 

advantageous,'  p.  76a,  to  *  found  farther  on/  of   the  instrument  and  other  circumstanceti 

p.  76c,  has  been  inserted,  both  in  accordance  I  did  not  pursue  the  investigation.    Mr.  A.  J. 

with  the  4th  German  edition. — Translator,]  Hipkins  informs  me  that  on  another  occasion 

f  [As  Prof.  Helmholtz  does  not  mention  he  got  out  of  the  &  string,  struck  at  ^  the 

the  striking  distance  of  the  hammer,  I  obtained  length,    the   6th,    7th,    8th,    and    9th    har> 

permission  from  Messrs.  Steinway  A  Sons,  at  monies,  as  in  the  experiments  mentioned  in 

their  London  house,  to  examine  the  c,  &  and  the  next  footnote, '  the  6th  and  7th  beautifully 

c"  strings  of  one  of  their  grand  pianos,  and  strong,  the  8th  and  9th  weaker  but  clear  and 

found  the  striking  distance  to  be  ^,  ^,  and  unmistakable.*    He  struck  with  the  hammer 

^  of  the  length  of  the  string  respectively,  always.    Observe  the  9th  harmonic  of  a  string 

I  did  not  measure  the  other  strings,  but  I  struck  with  a  pianoforte  hammer  at  its  node, 

observed  that   the  striking  distances  varied  or  J  its  length.— jfranskitor.] 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


CHAP.  V.   3. 


MUSICAL  TONES  OF  STEINGS. 


77 


because  it  has  a  node  at  that  point.  But  the  third  partial  tone  comes  out  forcibly, 
because  as  its  nodes  lie  at  ^  and  f  the  length  of  the  string  from  its  extremities, 
the  string  is  struck  half-way  between  these  two  nodes.  The  fourth  partial  has  its 
nodes  at  i,  }  (=^),  and  ^  the  length  of  the  string  from  its  extremity.  It  is  not 
heard,  because  the  point  of  excitement  corresponds  to  its  second  node.  The  sixth, 
eighth,  and  generally  the  partials  with  even  numbers  disappear  in  the  same  way,  but 
the  fifth,  seventh,  ninth,  and  the  other  partials  with  odd  numbers  are  heard.  By 
this  disappearance  of  tlie  evenly  numbered  partial  tones  when  a  string  is  struck  at  its 
middle,  the  quality  of  its  tone  becomes  peculiar,  and  essentially  different  from  that 
usually  heard  from  strings.  It  sounds  somewhat  hollow  or  nasal.  The  experi- 
ment is  easily  made  on  any  piano  when  it  is  opened  and  the  dampers  are  raised. 
The  middle  of  the  string  is  easily  found  by  trying  where  the  finger  must  be  laid 
to  bring  out  the  first  upper  partial  clearly  and  purely  on  striking  the  digital. 

If  the  string  is  struck  at  ^  its  length,  the  third,  sixth,  ninth,  &c.,  partials  f 
vanish.  This  also  gives  a  certain  amount  of  hollowness,  but  less  than  when  the 
string  is  struck  in  its  middle.  When  the  point  of  excitement  approaches  the  end 
of  the  string,  the  prominence  of  the  higher  upper  partials  is  favoured  at  the 
expense  of  the  prime  and  lower  upper  partial  tones,  and  the  sound  of  the  string 
becomes  poor  and  tinkling. 

In  pianofortes,  the  point  struck  is  about  |  to  |  the  length  of  the  string  from 
its  extremity,  for  the  middle  part  of  the  instrument.  We  must  therefore  assume 
that  this  place  has  been  chosen  because  experience  has  shewn  it  to  give  the  finest 
musical  tone,  which  is  most  suitable  for  harmonies.  The  selection  is  not  due  to 
theory.  It  results  from  attempts  to  meet  the  requirements,  of  artistically  trained 
eai's,  and  from  the  technical  experience  of  two  centuries.*    This  gives  particular 


*  [As  my  friend  Mr.  A.  J.  Hipkins,  of 
Broadwoods',  author  of  a  paper  on  the  '  History 
of  the  Pianoforte,*  in  the  Journal  of  the  Society 
of  Arts  (for  March  9,  18H3,  with  additions  on 
Sept.  21,  1883),  has  paid  great  attention  to  the 
archieology  of  the  pianoforte,  and  from  his 
position  at  Messrs.  Broadwoods'  has  the  best 
means  at  his  disposal  for  making  experiments, 
I  requested  him  to  favour  me  with  his  views 
upon  the  subject  of  the  striking  place  and 
harmonics  of  pianoforte  strings,  and  he  has 
obliged  me  with  the  following  observations : — 
'Harpsichords  and  spinets,  which  were  set 
in  vibration  by  quill  or  leather  plectra,  had 
no  fixed  point  for  plucking  the  strings.  It 
was  generally  from  about  ^  to  |  of  the  vibra- 
ting length,  and  although  it  had  been  observed 
by  Huyghens  and  the  Antwerp  harpsichord- 
maker  Jan  Couchet,  that  a  difference  of  quality 
of  tone  could  be  obtained  by  varying  the 
plucking  place  on  the  same  string,  which  led 
to  the  so-called  lute  stop  of  the  i8th  century, 
no  attempt  appears  to  have  been  made  to  gain 
a  uniform  striking  place  throughout  the  scale. 
Thus  in  the  latest  improved  spinet,  a  Hitch- 
cock, of  early  i8th  century,  in  my  possession, 
the  striking  place  of  the  c*8  varies  from  ^  to 
l«  and  in  the  latest  improved  harpsichord,  a 
Rirkman  of  1773,  also  in  my  possession,  the 
striking  distances  vary  from  i  to  y^  and  for 
the  lute  stop  from  jt  to  ^  of  the  string,  the 
longest  distances  in  the  bass  of  course,  but 
all  without  apparent  rule  or  proportion.  Nor 
was  any  attempt  to  gain  a  uniform  striking 
place  made  in  tiie  first  pianofortes.  Stein  of 
Augsburg  (the  favourite  pianoforte-maker  of 
Mozart,  and  of  Beethoven  in  his  virtuoso 
time)  knew  nothing  of  it,  at  least  in  his  early 
instruments.  The  great  length  of  the  bass 
strings  as  carried  out  on  the  single  belly- 
bridge  copied  from  the  harpsichord,  made  a 


reasonable  striking  place  for  that  part  of  the 
scale  impossible. 

*  John  Broadwood,  about  the  year  1788,  wan  ^ 
the  first  to  try  to  equalise  the  scale  in  tension 
and  striking  place.  He  called  in  scientific 
aid,  and  assisted  by  Signor  Gavallo  and  the 
then  Dr.  Gray  of  the  British  Museum,  he 
produced  a  divided  belly -bridge,  which  shorten- 
ing the  too  great  length  of  the  bass  strings, 
permitted  the  establishment  of  a  striking 
place,  which,  in  intention,  should  be  propor- 
tionate to  the  length  of  the  string  throughout. 
He  practically  adopted  a  ninth  of  the  vibrating 
length  of  the  string  for  his  striking  place, 
allowing  some  latitudie  in  the  treble.  This 
division  of  the  belly-bridge  becEme  universally 
adopted,  and  with  it  an  approximately  rational 
striking  place. 

•  Carl  Kiitzing  {Das  Wissenschaftliche  der 
Fortepiano-Baukunst,  1844,  p.  41)  was  enabled 
to  propound  from  experience,  that  i  of  the 
length  of  the  string  was  the  most  suitable  m 
distance  in  a  pianoforte  for  obtaining  the  best 
quality  of  tone  from  the  strings.  The  love  of 
noise  or  effect  has,  however,  inclined  makers  to 
shorten  distances,  particularly  in  the  trebles. 
Kiitzing  appears  to  have  met  with  ^th  in  some 
instances,  and  Helmholtz  has  adopted  that 
very  exceptional  measure  for  his  table  on 
p.  79c.  I  cannot  say  I  have  ever  met  with  a 
striking  place  of  this  long  distance  from  the 
wrestplank-bridge.  The  present  head  of  the 
firm  of  Broadwood  (Mr.  Henry  Fowler  Broad - 
wood)  has  arrived  at  the  same  conclusions  as 
Eutzing  with  respect  to  the  superiority  of  the 
^th  distance,  and  has  introduced  it  in  his 
pianofortes.  At  Ath  the  hammers  have  to  be 
softer  to  get  a  like  quality  of  tone ;  an  equal 
system  of  tension  being  presupposed. 

'According  to  Young's  law,  which  Helm- 
holtz by  experiment  confirms,  the  impact  of 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


78  MUSICAL  TONES  OF  STRINGS.  pabt  i. 

interest  to  the  mvestigation  of  the  composition  of  musical  tones  for  this  point  of 
excitement.  An  essential  advantage  in  the  choice  of  this  position  seems  to  be 
that  the  seventh  and  ninth  partial  tones  disappear  or  at  least  become  very  weak. 
These  are  the  first  in  the  series  of  partial  tones  which  do  not  belong  to  the  major 
chord  of  the  prime  tone.  Up  to  the  sixth  partial  we  have  only  Octaves,  Fifths, 
and  major  Thirds  of  the  prime  tone ;  the  seventh  is  nearly  a  minor  Seventh,  the 
ninth  a  major  Second  of  the  prime.  Hence  these  will  not  fit  into  the  major 
chord.  Experiments  on  pianofortes  shew  that  when  the  string  is  struck  by  the 
hammer  and  touched  at  its  nodes,  it  is  easy  to  bring  out  the  six  first  partial  tones 
(at  least  on  the  strings  of  the  middle  and  lower  octaves),  but  that  it  is  either  not 
possible  to  bring  out  the  seventh,  eighth,  and  ninth  at  all,  or  that  we  obtain  at 
best  very  weak  and  imperfect  results.  The  difficulty  here  is  not  occasioned  by  the 
incapacity  of  the  string  to  form  such  short  vibrating  sections,  for  if  instead  of  striking 

%  the  digital  we  pluck  the  string  nearer  to  its  end,  and  damp  the  corresponding 
nodes,  the  seventh,  eighth,  ninth,  nay  even  the  tenth  and  eleventh  partial  may  be 
clearly  and  brightly  produced.  It  is  only  in  the  upper  octaves  that  the  strings  are 
too  short  and  stiff  to  form  the  high  upper  partial  tones.  For  these,  several  instru- 
ment-makers place  the  striking  point  near^  to  the  extremity,  and  thus  obtain  a 
brighter  and  more  penetrating  tone.  The  upper  partii^  of  these  strings,  which 
their  stiffness  renders  it  difficult  to  bring  out,  are  thus  &voured  as  against  the 
prime  tone.  A  similarly  brighter  tone,  but  at  the  same  time  a  thinner  and  poorer 
one,  can  be  obtained  from  the  lower  strings  by  placing  a  bridge  nearer  the  striking 
point,  so  that  the  hammer  falls  at  a  point  less  than  4  of  the  effective  length  of  the 
string  from  its  extremity. 

While  on  the  one  hand  the  tone  can  be  rendered  more  tinkling,  shrill,  and 
acute,  by  striking  the  string  with  hard  bodies,  on  the  other  hand  it  can  be  rendered 
duller,  Uiat  is,  the  prime  tone  may  be  made  to  outweigh  the  upper  partials,  by 

f  striking  it  with  a  soft  and  heavy  hammer,  as,  for  example,  a  little  iron  hammer 
covered  with  a  thick  sheet  of  india-rubber.  The  strings  of  the  lower  octaves  then 
produce  a  much  fuller  but  duller  tone.  To  compare  the  different  qualities  of  tone 
thus  produced  by  using  hammers  of  different  constructions,  care  must  be  taken 
always  to  strike  the  string  at  the  same  distance  from  the  end  as  it  is  struck  by  the 
proper  hammer  of  the  instrument,  as  otherwise  the  results  would  be  mixed  up  with 
the  changes  of  quality  depending  on  altering  the  striking  point.  These  circum- 
stances are  of  course  well  known  to  the  instrument-makers,  because  they  have 

the  hammer  abolishes  the  node  of  the  striking  diately  after  production,  they  last  mnoh  longer 

place,  and  with  it  the  partial  belonging  to  it  and  are  much  brighter. 

throughoat  the  string.    I  do  not  find,  however,  *  I  do  not  think  the  treble  strings  are  from 

that  the  hammer  striking  at  the  ^th  elimi-  shortness  and  stiiffness  incapable  of  forming 

nates  the  8th  partial.    It  is  as  audible,  when  high  proper  tones.    If  it  were  so  the  notes 

touched  as  an  harmonic,  as  the  9th  and  higher  would  be  of  a  very  different  quality  of  tone  to 

parti^.    It  is  easy,  on  a  long  string  of  say  that  which  they  are  found  to  have.    Owing  to 

m  from  25  to  45  inches,  to  obtain  the  series  of  the  very  acute  pitch  of  these  tones  our  ears 

upper  partials  up  to  the   fifteenth.     On  a  cannot  follow  Uiem,  but   their  existence  is 

string  of  45  inches  I  have  obtained  as  far  as  proved  by  the  fact  that  instrument-makers 

the  23rd  harmonic,  the  diameter  of  the  wire  often  bring  their  treble  striking  place  very 

being  1-17  mm.  or  'o;  inches,  and  the  tension  near  the  wrestplank-bridge  in  order  to  secure 

being    71    kilogrammes    or   156*6   lbs.     The  a  brilliant  tone  effect,  or  ring,  by  tiie  pre- 

partials  diminish  in  intensity  with  the  re-  ponderance  of  these  harmonics, 
duction  of  the  vibrating  length;  the  2nd  is  *The    clavichord    differs     entirely    from 

stronger  than  the  3rd,  and  the  3rd  than  the  hammer  and  plectrum  keyboard  instruments 

4th,  &c.    Up  to  the  7th  a  good  harmonic  note  in  the  note  being  started  from  the  end,  the 

can  always  be  brought  out.    After  the  8th,  as  tangent  (brass  pin)  which  stops  the  string 

Helmholtz  says,  the  higher  partials  are  all  being  also  the  means  of  exciting  the  sound, 

comparatively    weak  and    become   gradually  But  the  thin  brass  wires  readily  break  up  into 

fainter.    To  strengthen  them  we  may  use  a  segments  of  short  recurrence,  the  bass  wires, 

narrower   harder   hammer.     To    hear    them  which  are  most  indistinct,  being  helped  in  the 

with  an  ordinary  hammer  it  is  necessary  to  latest  instruments  by  lighter  octave  strings^ 

excite  them  by  a  firm  blow  of  the  hand  upon  which  serve  to  make  the  fundamental  tones 

the  finger-key  and  to  continue  to  hold  it  down,  apparent.'    See  also  the  last  note,  p.  76<f ,  and 

They  sing  out  quite  clearly  and  last  a  very  App.  XX.  sect.  '^.—Translators] 


sensible  time.    On  removing  the  stop  imme- 


Digitized  by 


Google 


CHAP.  V.  3. 


MUSICAL  TONES  OP  STBINGS. 


79 


themselves  selected  heavier  and  softer  hammers  for  the  lower,  and  lighter  and 
harder  for  the  upper  octaves.  But  when  we  see  that  they  have  not  given  more 
than  a  certain  weight  to  the  hammers  and  have  not  increased  it  sufficiently  to 
reduce  the  intensity  of  the  upper  partial  tones  still  furtlier,  we  feel  convinced  that 
a  musically  trained  ear  prefers  that  an  instrument  to  be  used  for  rich  combinations 
of  harmony  should  possess  a  quality  of  tone  which  contains  upper  partials  with  a 
certain  amount  of  strength.  In  this  respect  the  composition  of  the  tones  of 
pianoforte  strings  is  of  great  interest  for  the  whole  theory  of  music.  In  no  other 
instrument  is  there  so  wide  a  field  for  alteration  of  quality  of  tone;  in  no  other, 
then,  was  a  musical  ear  so  unfettered  in  the  choice  of  a  tone  that  would  meet  its 
wishes. 

As  I  have  already  observed,  the  middle  and  lower  octaves  of  pianoforte  strings 
generally  allow  the  six  first  partial  tones  to  be  clearly  produced  by  striking  the 
digital,  and  the  three  first  of  them  are  strong,  the  fifth  and  sixth  distinct,  but  much  f 
weaker.  The  seventh,  eighth,  and  ninth  are  eliminated  by  the  position  of  the 
striking  point.  Those  higher  than  the  ninth  are  always  very  weak.  For  closer 
comparison  I  subjoin  a  table  in  which  the  intensities  of  the  partial  tones  of  a  string 
for  di£ferent  methods  of  striking  have  been  theoretically  calculated  firom  the 
fonnulae  developed  in  the  Appendix  V.  The  effect  of  the  stroke  of  a  hammer 
depends  on  the  length  of  time  for  which  it  touches  the  string.  This  time  is  given 
in  the  table  in  fractions  of  the  periodic  time  of  the  prime  tone.  To  this  is  added 
a  calculation  for  strings  plucked  by  the  finger.  The  striking  point  is  always 
assumed  to  be  at  |  of  the  length  of  the  string  from  its  extremity. 


Theoretical  Intensity  of  the  Partial  Tones  of  Strings. 


striking  point  at  |  of  the  length  of  the  string 

Stmck  by  a  hammer  which  touches  the  string  for 

Nnniber  of 

the  Partial 

Tone 

Excited  by 
Plucking 

0 
e" 

1        tV 

:  the  periodic  tiix 

e  of  the  prime  to 

le 

Struck  by  a 

perfect  hard 

Hammer 

I 

100 

100 

100 

100 

100 

100 

2 

8i-2 

997 

189-4 

249 

2857 

3247 

3 

561 

8-9 

107-9 

242-9 

357-0 

504-9 

4 

31-6 

2-3 

17-3 

1 18-9 

259-8 

504-9 

5 

13^ 

1*2 

0 

26-1 

108-4 

3247 

6 

a-8 

001 

o-s 

1-3 

i8-8 

loo-o 

7 

0 

0 

0 

0 

0 

0 

If 


For  easier  comparison  the  intensity  of  the  prime  tone  has  been  throughout 
assumed  as  100.  I  have  compared  the  calculated  intensity  of  the  upper  partials 
with  their  force  on  the  grand  pianoforte  already  mentioned,  and  found  that  the 
first  series,  under  ^,  suits  for  about  the  neighbourhood  of  0".  In  higher  parts  of  ^ 
the  instrument  the  upper  partials  were  much  weaker  than  in  this  column.  On 
striking  the  digital  for  c",  I  obtained  a  powerful  second  partial  and  an  almost  in- 
audible third.  The  second  column,  marked  -^,  corresponded  nearly  to  the  region  of 
g\  the  second  and  third  partials  were  very  strong,  the  fourth  partial  was  weak. 
The  third  column,  inscribed  -^,  corresponds  with  the  deeper  tones  from  &  down- 
wards ;  here  the  four  first  partials  are  strong,  and  the  fifth  weaker.  In  the  next 
column,  under  1^,  the  third  partial  tone  is  stronger  than  the  second ;  there  was 
no  corresponding  note  on  the  pianoforte  which  I  examined.  With  a  perfectly  hard 
hammer  the  third  and  fourth  partials  have  the  same  strength,  and  are  stronger 
than  all  the  others.  It  results  from  the  calculations  in  the  above  table  that  piano- 
forte tones  in  the  middle  and  lower  octaves  have  their  fundamental  tone  weaker 
than  the  first,  or  even  than  the  two  first  upper  partials.  This  can  also  be  con- 
firmed by  a  comparison  with  the  effects  of  plucked  strings.  For  the  latter  the 
second  partial  is  weaker  than  the  first;    and  it  will  be  found  that  the  prime 

Digitized  by  VjOOQlC 


8o  MUSICAL  TONES  OF  BOWED  INSTRUMENTS.  part  i. 

tone  is  much  more  distinct  in  tlie  tones  of  pianoforte  strings  when  plucked  by  the 
finger,  them  when  struck  by  the  hammer. 

Although,  as  is  shewn  by  the  mechanism  of  the  upper  octaves  on  pianofortes, 
it  is  possible  to  produce  a  compound  tone  in  which  the  prime  is  predominant, 
makers  have  preferred  arranging  the  method  of  striking  the  lower  strings  in  such 
a  way  as  to  preserve  the  five  or  six  first  partials  distinctly,  and  to  give  the  second 
and  third  greater  intensity  than  the  prime. 

Tliirdly,  as  regards  the  thickness  and  material  of  the  strings.  Very  rigid 
strings  will  not  form  any  very  high  upper  partials,  because  they  cannot  readily 
assume  inflections  in  opposite  directions  within  very  short  sections.  This  is  easily 
observed  by  stretching  two  strings  of  different  thicknesses  on  a  monochord  and 
endeavouring  to  produce  their  high  upper  partial  tones.  We  always  succeed  much 
better  with  the  thinner  than  with  the  thicker  string.  To  produce  very  high  upper 
^  partial  tones,  it  is  preferable  to  use  strings  of  extremely  fine  wire,  such  as  gold  lace 
makers  employ,  and  when  they  are  excited  in  a  suitable  manner,  as  for  example  by 
plucking  or  striking  with  a  metal  point,  these  high  upper  parfcials  may  be  heard  in 
the  compound  itself.  The  numerous  high  upper  pai-tials  which  he  close  to  each 
other  in  the  scale,  give  that  peculiar  high  inharmonious  noise  which  we  are 
accustomed  to  call '  tinkUng.'  From  the  eighth  partial  tone  upwards  these  simple 
tones  are  less  than  a  whole  Tone  apart,  and  from  the  fifteenth  upwards  less  than  a 
Semitone.  They  consequently  form  a  series  of  dissonant  tones.  On  a  string  of 
the  finest  iron  wire,  such  as  is  used  in  the  manufJEUsture  of  artificial  flowers,  700 
centimetres  (22*97  feet)  long,  I  was  able  to  isolate  the  eighteenth  partial  tone.  The 
peculiarity  of  the  tones  of  the  zither  depends  on  the  presence  of  these  tinkling 
upper  partials,  but  the  series  does  not  extend  so  flEbr  as  that  just  mentioned,  because 
the  strings  are  shorter. 

Strings  of  gut  are  much  lighter  than  metal  strings  of  the  same  compactness, 
^  and  hence  produce  higher  partial  tones.  The  difference  of  their  musical  quality 
depends  partly  on  this  circumstance  and  partly  on  the  inferior  elasticity  of  the  gut, 
which  damps  their  partials,  especially  their  higher  partials,  much  more  rapidly. 
The  tone  of  plucked  cat-gut  strings  {guitar,  harp)  is  consequently  much  lesa 
tinkling  than  that  of  metal  strings. 

4.  Mtisical  Tones  of  Bowed  Instruments, 

No  complete  mechanical  theory  can  yet  be  given  for  the  motion  of  strings 
excited  by  the  violin-bow,  because  the  mode  in  which  the  bow  afifects  the  motion 
of  the  string  is  unknown.  But  by  applying  a  peculiar  method  of  observation, 
proposed  in  its  essential  features  by  the  I^Vench  physicist  Lissajous,  I  have  found 
it  possible  to  observe  the  vibrational  form  of  individual  points  in  a  violin  string, 
and  from  this  observed  form,  which  is  comparatively  very  simple,  to  calculate  the 
^  whole  motion  of  the  string  and  the  intensity  of  the  upper  partial  tones. 

Look  through  a  hand  magnifying  glass  consisting  of  a  strong  convex  lens,  at 

any  small  bright  object,  as  a  grain  of  starch  reflecting  a  flame,  and  appearing  as  a 

fine  point  of  Ught.    Move  the  lens  about  while  the  point  of  light  remains  at  rest, 

and  the  point  itself  will  appear  to  move.  In  the  apparatus  I  have  employed,  which 

is  shewn  in  fig.  22  opposite,  this  lens  is  fastened  to  the  end  of  one  prong  of  the 

tuning-fork  G,  and  marked  L.     It  is  in  faxst  a  combination  of  two  achromatic 

lenses,  like  those  used  for  the  object-glasses  of  microscopes.     These  two  lenses 

may  be  used  alone  as  a  doublet,  or  be  combined  with  others.      When  more 

f     magnifying  power  is  required,  we  can  introduce  behind  the  metal  plate  A  A,  which 

I     carries  the  fork,  the  tube  and  eye-piece  of  a  microscope,  of  which  the  doublet  then 

forms  the  object-glass.     This  instrument  may  be  called  a  vibration  microscope. 

.     When  it  is  so  arranged  that  a  fixed  luminous  point  may  be  clearly  seen  through  it, 

and  the  fork  is  set  in  vibration,  the  doublet  L  moves  periodically  up  and  down  in 

pendular  vibrations.    The  observer,  however,  appears  to  see  the  luminous  point 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


CHAP.  V.  4.    MUSICAL  TONES   OF  BOWED  INSTRUMENTS. 


8i 


itself  vibrate,  and,  since  the  separate  vibrations  succeed  each  other  so  rapidly  that 
the  impression  on  the  eye  cannot  die  away  daring  the  time  of  a  whole  vibration, 
the  path  of  the  luminous  point  appears  as  a  fixed  straight  line,  increasing  in  length 
with  the  excursions  of  the  fork.* 

The  grain  of  starch  which  reflects  the  Hght  to  be  seen,  is  then  fastened  to  the 
resonant  body  whose  vibrations  we  intend  to  observe,  in  such  a  way  that  the  grain 
moves  backwards  and  forwards  horizontally,  while  the  doublet  moves  up  and  down 
vertically.  When  both  motions  take  place  at  once,  the  observer  sees  the  real 
horizontal  motion  of  the  luminous  point  combined  with  its  apparent  vertical  motion, 
and  the  combination  results  in  an  apparent  curvilinear  motion.  The  field  of  vision 
in  the  microscope  then  shews  an  apparently  steady  and  unchangeable  bright 

Frn.  22. 


/ 


curve,  when  either  the  periodic  times  of  the  vibrations  of  the  grain  of  starch  and  ^ 
of  the  tuning-fork  are  exactly  equal,  or  one  is  exactly  two  or  three  or  four  times  as 
great  as  the  other,  because  in  this  case  the  luminous  point  passes  over  exactly  the 
same  path  every  one  or  every  two,  three,  or  four  vibrations.  If  these  ratios  of  the 
vibrational  numbers  are  not  exactly  perfect,  the  curves  alter  slowly,  and  the  effect 
to  the  eye  is  as  if  they  were  drawn  on  the  surface  of  a  transparent  cylinder  which 
slowly  revolved  on  its  axis.  This  slow  displacement  of  the  apparent  curves  is  not 
disadvantageous,  as  it  allows  the  observer  to  see  them  in  different  positions.  But 
if  the  ratio  of  the  pitch  numbers  of  the  observed  body  and  of  the  fork  differs  too 


*  The  end  of  the  other  prong  of  the  fork 
is  thickened  to  counterbalance  the  weight  of 
the  doublet.  The  iron  loop  B  which  is  clamped 
on  to  one  prong  serves  to  alter  the  pitch  of 
the  fork  slightly;  we  flatten  the  pitch  by 
moving  the  loop  towards  the  end  of  the  prong. 


E  is  an  electro-magnet  by  which  the  fork  is 
kept  in  constant  uniform  vibration  on  passing 
intermittent  electrical  currents  through  its 
wire  coils,  as  will  be  described  more  in  detail 
in  Chapter  VI. 


Digitized  by  G(90gle 


82  MUSICAL  TONES  OP  BOWED  INSTRUMENTS.  part  i. 

much  from  one  expressible  by  small  whole  numbers,  the  motion  of  the  curve  is  too 
rapid  for  the  eye  to  follow  it,  and  all  becomes  confusion. 

If  the  vibration  microscope  has  to  be  used  for  observing  the  motion  of  a  violin 
string,  the  luminous  point  must  be  attached  to  that  string.  This  is  done  by  first 
blackening  the  required  spot  on  the  string  with  ink,  and  when  it  is  dry,  rubbing  it 
over  with  wax,  and  powdering  this  with  starch  so  that  a  few  grains  remain  sticking. 
The  violin  is  then  fixed  with  its  strings  in  a  vertical  direction  opposite  the  micro- 
scope, so  that  the  luminous  reflection  from  one  of  the  grains  of  starch  can  be 
clearly  seen.  The  bow  is  drawn  across  the  strings  in  a  direction  parallel  to  the 
prongs  of  the  fork.  Every  point  in  the  string  then  moves  horizontally,  and  on 
setting  the  fork  in  motion  at  the  same  time,  the  observer  sees  the  peculiar 
vibrational  curves  already  mentioned.  For  the  purposes  of  observation  I  used  the 
a'  string,  which  I  tuned  a  little  higher,  as  V^,  so  that  it  was  exactly  two  Octaves 
"'  higher  than  the  tuning-fork  of  the  microscope,  which  sounded  B^. 

In  fig.  23  are  shewn  the  resulting  vibrational  curves  as  seen  in  the  vibration 
microscope.    The  straight  horizontal  lines  in  the  figures,  atoa,  btob,  ctoe 

yiQ.  23. 


\  shew  the  apparent  path  of  the  observed  luminous  point,  before  it  had  itself  been 
set  in  vibration ;  the  curves  and  zigzags  in  the  same  figures,  shew  the  apparent 
path  of  the  luminous  point  when  it  also  was  made  to  move.  By  their  side,  in  A, 
B,  C,  the  same  vibrational  forms  are  exhibited  according  to  the  methods  used  in 
Chapters  I.  and  11.,  the  lengths  of  the  horizontal  line  being  directly  proportional 
to  the  corresponding  lengths  of  tvme^  whereas  in  figures  a  to  a,  b  to  b,  c  to  c,  the 
horizontal  lengths  are  proportional  to  the  eoccursions  of  the  vibrating  microscope. 

^  A,  and  a  to  a,  shew  the  vibrational  curves  for  a  tuning-fork,  that  is  for  a  simple 
pendular  vibration  ;  B  and  b  to  b  those  of  the  middle  of  a  violin  string  in  unison 
with  the  fork  of  the  vibration  microscope ;  G  and  c,  c,  those  for  a  string  which  was 
tuned  an  Octave  higher.  We  may  imagine  the  figures  a  to  a,  b  to  b,  and  c  to  c,  to 
be  formed  from  the  figures  A,  B,  G,  by  supposing  the  surface  on  which  these  are 
drawn  to  be  wrapped  round  a  transparent  cylinder  whose  circumference  is  of  the 
same  length  as  the  horizontal  line.  The  curve  drawn  upon  the  surface  of  the 
cylinder  must  then  be  observed  from  such  a  point,  that  the  horizontal  line  which 
when  wrapped  round  the  cylinder  forms  a  circle,  appears  perspectively  as  a  single 
straight  line.  The  vibrational  curve  A  wiU  then  appear  in  the  forms  a  to  a,  B  in 
the  forms  b  to  b,  G  in  the  forms  c  to  c.  When  the  pitch  of  the  two  vibrating 
bodies  is  not  in  an  exact  harmonic  ratio,  this  imaginary  cylinder  on  which  the 
vibrational  curves  are  drawn,  appears  to  revolve  so  that  the  forms  a  to  a,  &c.,  are 
assumed  in  succession. 

It  is  now  easy  to  rediscover  the  forms  A,  B,  G,  from  the  forms  a  to  a,  b  to  b« 

Digitized  by  V^OOQIC 


CHAP.  Y.  4.    MUSICAL  TONES  OF  BOWED  INSTRUMENTS.  83 

and  c  to  c,  and  as  the  former  give  a  more  intelligible  image  of  the  motion  of  the 
string  than  the  latter,  the  cnrves,  which  are  seen  as  if  they  were  traced  on  the 
snrface  of  a  cylinder,  will  be  drawn  as  if  their  trace  had  been  unrolled  from  the 
cylinder  into  a  plane  figure  like  A,  B,  G.  The  meaning  of  our  vibrational  curves 
will  then  precisely  correspond  to  the  similar  curves  in  preceding  chapters.  When 
four  vibrations  of  the  violin  string  correspond  to  one  vibration  of  the  fork  (as  in 
our  experiments,  where  the  fork  gave  ^  and  the  string  &]>,  p.  82a),  so  that 
four  waves  seem  to  be  traced  on  the  surface  of  the  imi^ginary  cylinder,  and  when 
moreover  they  are  made  to  rotate  slowly  and  are  thus  viewed  in  different  positions, 
it  is  not  at  aJl  difGicult  to  draw  them  from  inunediate  inspection  as  if  they  had 
been  rolled  off  on  to  a  plane,  for  the  middle  jags  have  then  nearly  the  same 
appearance  on  the  cylinder  as  if  they  were  traced  on  a  plane. 

The  figures  23  B  and  23  C  (p.  Sib),  inmiediately  give  the  vibrational  forms  for 
the  middle  of  a  violin  string,  when  the  bow  bites  well,  and  the  prime  tone  of  the  f 
string  is  folly  and  powerfully  produced.  It  is  easily  seen  that  these  vibrational 
forms  are  essentially  different  from  that  of  a  simple  vibration  (fig.  23,  A).  When 
the  point  is  taken  nearer  the  ends  of  the  string  the  vibrational  figure  is  shewn  in 
fig.  24,  A,  and  the  two  sections  afi,  Py,  of  any  wave,  are  to  one  another  as  the  two 
sections  of  the  string  which  lie  on  either  side  of  the  observed  point.    In  the  figure 

Fig.  24. 


this  ratio  is  3  :  i,  the  point  being  at  ^  the  length  of  the  string  from  its  extremity. 
Close  to  the  end  of  the  string  the  form  is  as  in  fig.  24,  B.    The  short  lengths  of 
line  in  the  figure  have  been  made  fEunt  because  the  corresponding  motion  of  the  ^ 
Imninous  point  is  so  rapid  that  they  often  become  invisible,  and  the  thicker  lengths 
are  alone  seen.* 

These  figures  shew  that  every  point  of  the  string  between  its  two  extremities 
vibrates  with  a  constant  velocity.  For  the  middle  point,  the  velocity  of  ascent  is 
equal  to  that  of  descent.  If  tiie  violin  bow  is  used  near  the  right  end  of  the 
string  descending,  the  velocity  of  descent  on  the  right  half  of  the  string  is  less 
than  that  of  ascent,  and  the  more  so  the  nearer  to  the  end.  On  the  left  half  of 
the  string  the  converse  takes  place.  At  the  place  of  bowing  the  velocity  of  descent 
appears  to  be  equal  ta  that  of  the  violin  bow.  During  the  greater  part  of  each 
vibration  the  string  here  clings  to  the  bow,  and  is  carried  on  by  it ;  then  it  suddenly 
detaches  itself  and  rebounds,  whereupon  it  is  seized  by  other  points  in  the  bow  and 
again  carried  fbrward.f 

Our  present  purpose  is  chiefly  to  determine  the  upper  partial  tones.  The 
vibrational  forms  of  the  individual  points  of  the  string  being  known,  the  intensity  f 
of  each  of  the  partial  tones  can  be  completely  calculated.  The  necessary  mathe- 
matical formula  are  developed  in  Appendix  YI.  The  following  is  the  result  of  the 
calculation.  When  a  string  excite4  by  a  violin  bow  speaks  well,  all  the  upper 
partial  tones  which  can  be  formed^by  a  string  of  its  degree  of  rigidity,  are  present, 
and  their  intensity  diminishes  d^  their  pitch  increases.  The  amplitude  and  the 
intensity  of  the  second  partial  is  one-fourth  of  that  of  the  prime  tone,  that  of  the 

*  [Dr.  Hnggins,  FJft.S.,  on  ezperimentmg,  string  has  been  given  by  Herr  Qem.  Neumann 

finds  it  probable  that  under  the   bow,  the  in  the  Proceedings  {SitnmgsberiokU)  of  the 

reUtive  vdooity  of   descent  to  that  of  the  J.  and  R.  Academy  at  Vienna,  mathematical 

lebonnd  of  tile  string,  or  ascent,  is  influenced  and  physical  class,  vol.  Izi.  p.  89.   He  fastened 

by  &e  tension  of  tiie  hairs  of  the  bow. —  bits  of  wire  in  the  fonn  of  a  oomb  to  the  bow 

TramUUor.l  itself.     On  looking  through  this  grating. at 

t  These   facts   suffice    to  determine   the  the  string   the  observer  sees    a   system    of 

complete    motion   of    bowed    strings.      Bee  rectilinear  zigaag  lines.    The  conclusions  as 

Appendix  YL     A  much  simpler  method  of  to  the  mode  of  motion  of  the  string  agree 

obeerving  the   vibrational  ftnn   of   a  violin  with  those  given  above. 


Digitized  by 


Cdogle 


84  MUSICAL  TONES  OP  BOWED  INSTRUMENTS.         pabt  i. 

third  partial  a  ninth,  that  of  the  fourth  a  sixteenth,  and  so  on.  This  is  the  same 
scale  of  intensity  as  for  the  partial  tones  of  a  string  plucked  in  its  middle,  with 
this  exception,  that  in  the  latter  case  the  evenly  numbered  partials  all  disappear, 
whereas  they  are  all  present  when  the  bow  is  used.  The  upper  partials  in  the 
compound  tone  of  a  violin  are  heard  easily  and  will  be  found  to  be  strong  in  sound 
if  they  have  been  first  produced  as  so-called  harmonics  on  the  string,  by  bowing 
lightly  while  gently  touching  a  node  of  the  required  partial  tone.  The  strings  of 
a  violin  will  allow  the  harmonics  to  be  produced  as  high  as  the  sixth  partial  tone 
with  ease,  and  with  some  difficulty  even  up  to  the  tenth.  The  lower  tones  speak 
best  when  the  string  is  bowed  at  from  one-tenth  to  one-twelfth  the  length  of  the 
vibrating  portion  of  the  string  from  its  extremity.  For  the  higher  harmonics 
where  the  sections  are  smaller,  the  strings  must  be  bowed  at  about  one-fourth  or 
one-sixth  of  their  vibrating  length  from  the  end.* 

^  The  prime  in  the  compound  tones  of  bowed  iostruments  is  comparatively  more 
powerful  than  in  those  produced  on  a  pianoforte  or  guitar  by  striking  or  plucking 
the  strings  near  to  their  extremities ;  the  first  upper  partials  are  comparatively 
weaker ;  but  the  higher  upper  partials  from  the  sixth  to  about  the  tenth  are  much 
more  distinct,  and  give  these  tones  their  cutting  character. 

The  fimdamental  form  of  the  vibrations  of  a  violin  string  just  described,  is, 
when  the  string  speaks  well,  tolerably  independent  of  the  place  of  bowing,  at  least 
in  all  essential  features.  It  does  not  in  any  respect  alter,  like  the  vibrational  form 
of  struck  or  plucked  strings,  according  to  the  position  of  the  point  excited.  Yet 
there    are  certain  obser-  ^^^  ^^ 

vable  differences  of  the 
vibrational  figure  which 
depend  upon  the  bowing 
point.  Little  crumples  are 

m  usually  perceived  on  the 
lines  of  the  vibrational 
figure,  as  in  fig.  25,  which 
increase  in  breadth  and  height  the  further  the  bow  is  removed  from  the  extremity 
of  the  string.  When  we  bow  at  a  node  of  one  of  the  higher  upper  partials 
which  is  near  the  bridge,  these  crumples  are  simply  reduced  by  the  absence  of 
that  part  of  the  normal  motion  of  the  string  which  depends  on  the  partial  tones 
having  a  node  at  that  place.  When  the  observation  on  the  vibrational  form  is 
made  at  one  of  the  other  nodes  belonging  to  the  deepest  tone  which  is  elimi- 
nated, none  of  these  crumples  are  seen.  Thus  if  the  string  is  bowed  at  |th, 
or  |ths,  or  fths,  or  |ths,  &c.,  of  its  length  from  the  bridge,  the  vibrational 
figure  is  simple,  as  in  fig.  24  (p.  836).  But  if  we  observe  between  two  nodes, 
the  crumples  appear  as  in  fig.  25.  Variations  in  the  quality  of  tone  partly 
depend  upon  this  condition.  When  the  violin  bow  is  brought  too  near  the 
finger  board,  the  end  of  which  is  Jtth  the  length  of  the  string  from  the  bridge, 
^  the  5th  or  6th  partial  tone,  which  is  generally  distinctly  audible,  will  be  absent. 
The  tone  is  thus  rendered  duller.  The  usual  place  of  bowing  is  at  about  Vv^ 
of  the  length  of  the  string ;  for  piano  passages  it  is  somewhat  further  from 
the  bridge  and  for  forte  somewhat  nearer  to  it.  If  the  bow  is  brought  near  the 
bridge,  and  at  the  same  time  but  lightly  pressed,  another  alteration  of  quality 
occurs,  which  is  readily  seen  on  the  vibrational  figure.    A  mixtiure  is  formed  of 


*  (The  position  of  the  finger  for  prodndng  near  the  not,    out  of    165  mm.  the  actual 

the  harmonio  is  often  slightly  different  from  half  length  of  the  strings.    These  differences 

that    theoretici^y    assigned.     Dr.    Hnggins,  most    therefore   be   due    to   some    imperfec- 

F.B.S.,  kindly  tried  for  me  the  position  of  tions  of  the  strings  themselves.    Dr.  Huggins 

the  Octave  harmonio  on  the  four  strings  of  finds  that  there  is  a  space  of  a  quarter  of 

his  Stradivari,  a  mark  with  Chinese  white  an  inch  at  any  point  of  which  the   Octave 

being  made  under  his  finger  on  the   finger  harmonic  may  be  brought  out,  but  the  quality 

board.     Besult,    ist   and   4th   string  exact,  of  tone  is  best  at  the  points  named  above.— 

2ud  string  3  mm.,  and  3rd  string  5  mm.  too  Translator.] 

Digitized  by  V^OOQIC 


CHAP.  V.  4.     MUSICAL  TONES  OF  BOWED  INSTRUMENTS.  85 

the  4>rimd  tone  and  first  harmonic  of  the  string.  By  light  and  rapid  bowing, 
namely  at  about  ^^th  of  the  length  of  the  string  from  the  bridge,  we  sometimes 
obtain  the  upper  Octave  of  the  prime  tone  by  itself,  a  node  being  formed  in  the 
middle  of  the  string.  On  bowing  more  firmly  the  prime  tone  immediately  sounds. 
Intermediately  the  higher  Octave  may  mix  with  it  in  any  proportion.  This  is 
immediately  recognised  in  the  vibrational  figure.  Fig.  26  gives  the  corresponding 
series  of  forms.  It  is  seen  how  a  firesh  crest  appears  on  the  longer  side  of  the 
front  of  a  wave,  jutting  out  at  first  slightly,  then  more  strongly,  till  at  length  the 
crest  of  the  new  waves  are  as  high  as  those  of  the  old,  and  Uien  the  vibrational 
number  has  doubled,  and  the  pitch  has  passed  into  the  Octave  above.  The  quality 
of  the  lowest  tone  of  the  string  is  rendered  softer  and  brighter,  but  less  full  and 
powerful  when  the  intermixture  commences.  It  is  interesting  to  observe  the 
Tibrational  figure  while  httle  changes  are  made  in  the  style  of  bowing,  and  note 
how  the  resulting  slight  changes  of  quality  are  immediately  rendered  evident  by  ' 
very  distinct  changes  in  the  vibrational  figure  itself. 

The  vibrational  forms  just  described  may  be  maintained  in  a  uniformly  steady 
and  unchanged  condition  by  carefuUy  uniform  bowing.  The  instrument  has  then 
an  uninterrupted  and  pure  musical  quality  of  tone.  Any  scratching  of  the  bow  is 
inmiediately  shewn  by  sudden  jumps,  or  discontinuous  displacements  and  changes 
in  the  vibrational  figure.  If  the  scratching  continues,  the  eye  has  no  longer  time 
to  perceive  a  regular  figure.  The  scratching  noises  of  a  violin  bow  must  therefore 
be  regarded  as  irregular  interruptions  of  the  normal  vibrations  of  the  string, 
making  them  to  recommence  from  a  new  starting  point.    Sudden  jumps  in  the 

Fio.  26. 


vibrational  figure  betray  every  little  stumble  of  the  bow  which  the  ear  alone  would 
scarcely  observe.  Inferior  bowed  instruments  seem  to  be  distinguished  from  good 
ones  by  the  frequency  of  such  greater  or  smaller  irregularities  of  vibration.  On 
the  string  of  my  monochord,  which  was  only  used  for  the  occasion  as  a  bowed 
instmment,  great  neatness  of  bovring  was  required  to  preserve  a  steady  vibrational 
figure  lasting  long  enough  for  the  eye  to  apprehend  it ;  and  the  tone  was  rough  in 
quality,  accompanied  by  much  scratching.  With  a  very  good  modem  violin  made 
by  Bausch  it  was  easier  to  maintain  the  steadiness  of  the  vibrational  figure  for 
some  time ;  but  I  succeeded  much  better  with  an  old  Italian  violin  of  Guadanini, 
which  was  the  first  one  on  which  I  could  keep  the  vibrational  figure  steady  enough  % 
to  count  the  crumples.  This  great  uniformity  of  vibration  is  evidently  the  reason 
of  the  purer  tone  of  these  old  instruments,  since  every  little  irregularity  is  imme- 
diately felt  by  the  ear  as  a  roughness  or  scratchiness  in  the  quality  of  tone. 

An  appropriate  structure  of  the  instrument,  and  wood  of  the  most  perfect 
elasticity  procurable,  are  probably  the  important  conditions  for  regular  vibrations 
of  the  string,  and  when  these  are  present,  the  bow  can  be  easily  made  to  work 
uniformly.  This  allows  of  a  pure  flow  of  tone,  undisfigured  by  any  roughness*. 
On  the  other  hand,  when  the  vibrations  are  so  uniform  the  string  can  be  more 
vigorously  attacked  with  the  bow.  Good  instruments  consequently  allow  of  a  much 
more  powerful  motion  of  the  string,  and  the  whole  intensity  of  their  tone  can  be 
communicated  to  the  air  without  diminution,  whereas  the  friction  caused  by  any 
imperfection  in  the  elasticity  of  the  wood  destroys  part  of  the  motion.  Much  of 
the  advantages  of  old  violins  may,  however,  also  depend  upon  their  age,  and  espe- 
cially their  long  use,  both  of  which  cannot  but  act  favourably  on  the  elasticity  of 

Digitized  by  V^OOQ IC 


86  MUSICAL  TONES  OP  BOWED  INSTRUMENTS.         past  i. 

the  wood.  But  the  art  of  bowing  is  evidently  the  most  important  condition  of  all. 
How  delicately  this  must  be  cultivated  to  obtain  certainty  in  producing  a  very 
perfect  quality  of  tone  and  its  different  varieties,  cannot  be  more  clearly  demon- 
strated than  by  the  observation  of  vibrational  figures.  It  is  also  well  known  that 
great  players  can  bring  out  full  tones  from  even  indifferent  instruments. 

The  preceding  observations  and  conclusions  refer  to  the  vibrations  of  the  strings 
of  the  instrument  and  the  intensity  of  their  upper  partial  tones,  solely  in  so  feu:  as 
they  are  contained  in  the  compound  vibrational  movement  of  the  string.  But 
partial  tones  of  different  pitches  are  not  equally  well  communicated  to  the  air,  and 
hence  do  not  strike  the  ear  of  the  listener  with  precisely  the  same  degrees  of 
intensity  as  those  they  possess  on  the  string  itself.  They  are  communicated  to 
the  air  by  means  of  the  sonorous  body  of  the  instrument.  As  we  have  had 
already  occasion  to  remark,  vibrating  strings  do  not  directly  communicate  any 

f  sensible  portion  of  their  motion  to  the  air.  The  vibrating  strings  of  the  violin, 
in  the  first  place,  agitate  the  bridge  over  which  they  are  stretched.  This  stands 
on  two  feet  over  the  most  mobile  part  of  the  '  belly '  between  the  two  '/  holes.' 
One  foot  of  the  bridge  rests  upon  a  comparatively  firm  support,  namely,  the  *  sound- 
post,'  which  is  a  solid  rod  inserted  between  the  two  plates,  back  and  belly,  of  the 
instrument.  It  is  only  the  other  leg  which  agitates  the  elastic  wooden  plates,  and 
through  them  the  included  mass  of  air.* 

An  inclosed  mass  of  air,  like  that  of  the  violin,  vi61a,  and  violoncello,  bounded 
by  elastic  plates,  has  certain  proper  tones  which  may  be  evoked  by  blowing 
across  the  openings,  or  *  f  holes.*  The  violin  thus  treated  gives  c'  according  to 
Savart,  who  examined  instruments  made  by  Stradivari  (Stradiuarius).t  Zam- 
miner  found  the  same  tone  constant  on  even  imperfect  instruments.  For  the 
violoncello  Savart  found  on  blowing  over  the  holes  F,  and  Zamminer  G.t  Ac- 
cording to  Zamminer  the  sound-box  of  the  vi61a  (tenor)  is  tuned  to  be  a  Tone 

^  deeper  than  that  of  the  violin.  §  On  placing  the  ear  against  the  back  of  a  violin 
and  playing  a  scale  on  the  pianoforte,  some  tones  will  be  found  to  penetrate  the 
ear  with  more  force  than  others,  owing  to  the  resonance  of  the  instrument.    On  a 

*  [Thia  aoooont   is  not  quite    suffioieni.  agitation  transmitted  by  the  rod.*    In  short. 

Neither   leg  of  the  bridge  rests  exactly  on  the  touoh  rod  acts  as  a  soond-post  to  the 

the  sound-post,  becaase  it  is  found  that  this  finger.    The  place  of  least  vibration  of  the 

position  materially  injures  the  quality  of  tone,  belly  is  exactly  over  the  sound-post  and  of  the 

The  sound-post  is  a  little  in  the  rear  of  the  back  at  the  point  under  the  sound-post.    On 

leg  of  the  bridge  on  the  «"  string  side.    The  removing  the  sound-post,  or  covering  its  ends 

position  of  the  sound-post  with  regard  to  the  with  a  sheet  of  india-rubber,  which  did  not 

bridge  has  to  be  adjusted  for  each  individual  diminish  the  support,  the  tone  was  poor  and 

instrument.    Dr.  William  Huggins,  F.B.S.,  in  thin.    But  an  external  wooden  clamp  oonneet- 

his  paper  *  On  the  Function  of  the  Sound-post,  ing  belly  and  back  in  the  places  where  the 

and  on  the   Proportional   Thickness  of  the  sound-post  touches  them,  restored  the  tone.-^ 

Strings  of  the  Violin,'  read  May  24,   1883,  Translator.'] 

Proceedings  of  the  Royal  Society^  vol.  xxxv.  f  [Zanmiiner,    Die   Miisih^    1855,   voL   L 

«|T  pp.  241-248,  has  experimentally  investigated  p.  37,  says  d  of  256  vlb. — Translator.] 
^'  the  whole  action  of  the  sound-post,  and  finds  %  [Zamminer,  ibid.  p.  41,  and  adds  that 

that  its  main  function  is  to  convey  vibrations  judging  from  the  violin  the  resonance  shoold 

from  the  belly  to  the  back  of  the  violin,  in  be  Fff . — Translator,] 

addition  to  those  conveyed  by  the  sides.    The  §  [The  passage  referred  to  has  not  been 

(apparently  ornamental)  cuttings  in  the  bridge  found.     But    Zamminer  says,    p.  40,    *  The 

of  the  violin,  sift  the  two  sets  of  vibrations,  length  of  the  box  of  a  violin  is  13  Par.  inches, 

set  up  by  the  bowed  string  at  right  angles  to  and  of  the  vi61a  14  inches  5  lines.    Exactly 

each  other  and '  allow  those  only  or  mainly  to  in    inverse    ratio    stand  the  pitch   nombers 

pass  to  the  feet  which  would  be  efficient  in  470  (a  misprint  for  270  most  probably)  and 

setting  the  body  of  the  instrument  into  vibra-  241,  which  were  found  by  blowing  over  the 

tion.*    As  the  peculiar  shape  of  the  instru-  wind-holes  of  the  two  instruments.*    Now  the 

ment  rendered  strewing  of  sand  unavailable,  ratio  13  :  14^3  gives  182  cents,  and  the  ratio 

Pr.  Huggins  investigated  the  vibrations  by  241  :  270  gives    197   cents,  which   are   very 

means  of  a  *  touch  rod,*  consisting  of  '  a  small  nearly,  though  not  *  exactly '  the  same.    Hiis, 

round  stick  of  straight  grained  deal  a  few  however,  makes  the  resonance  of  the  violin 

inches  long;  the  forefinger  is  placed  on  one  270  vib.  and  not  256  vib.,  and  agrees  with  the 

end  and  the  other  end  is  put  lightly  in  contact  next  note.    I  got  a  good  resonance  with  a  fork 

with  the  vibrating  surface.    The  finger  soon  of  268  vib.  from  Dr.  Huggins's  violoncoUo  by 

becomes  very  sensitive  to  small  differences  of  Nicholas  about  ^.d.  if<)2.— Translator.] 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


OBAP.  T.  4.    MUSICAL  TONES  OF  BOWED  INSTBUMENTS. 


«7 


violin  made  by  Bansoh  two  tones  of  greatest  resonance  were  thus  discovered,  one 
between  d  and  c%  [between  264  and  280  vib.],  and  the  other  between  a'  and  &]> 
[between  440  and  466  vib.].  For  a  vi61a  (tenor)  I  found  the  two  tones  about  a 
Tone  deeper,  which  agrees  with  Zanuniner's  calculation.* 

The  consequence  of  this  peculiar  relation  of  resonance  is  that  those  tones  of 
the  strings  which  lie  near  the  proper  tones  of  the  inclosed  body  of  air,  must  be 
proportionably  more  reinforced.  This  is  clearly  perceived  on  botii  the  violin  and 
violoncello,  at  least  for  the  lowest  proper  tone,  when  the  corresponding  notes  are 
produced  on  the  strings.  They  sound  particularly  fuU,  and  the  prime  tone  of  these 
compounds  is  especially  prominent.  I  think  that  I  heard  this  also  for  al  on  the 
violin,  which  corresponds  to  its  higher  proper  tone. 

Since  the  lowest  tone  on  the  violin  is  ^,  the  only  upper  partials  of  its  musical 
tones  which  can  be  somewhat  reinforced  by  the  resonance  of  the  higher  proper 
tone  of  its  inclosed  body  of  air,  are  the  higher  octaves  of  its  three  deepest  notes.  «p 
But  the  prime  tones  of  its  higher  notes  will  be  reinforced  more  than  their  upper 
partials,  because  these  prime  tones  are  more  nearly  of  the  same  pitch  as  the 
proper  tones  of  the  body  of  air.  This  produces  an  effect  similar  to  that  of  the  con- 
struction of  the  hanmier  of  a  piano,  which  favours  the  upper  partials  of  the  deep 
notes,  and  weakens  those  of  the  higher  notes.  For  the  violoncello,  where  the  lowest 
string  gives  C,  the  stronger  proper  tone  of  the  body  of  air  is,  as  on  the  violin,  a 
Fourtti  or  a  Fifth  higher  than  the  pitch  of  the  lowest  string.  There  is  consequently 
a  similar  relation  between  the  fi&voured  and  unfavoured  partial  tones,  but  all  of 


*  [Throogh  the  kindness  of  Dr.  Haggins, 
F  JI.S.,  the  Bev.  H.  B.  Haweis,  and  the  violin- 
makers,  Messrs.  Hart,  Hill  ft  Withers,  I  was 
in  1880  enabled  to  examine  the  pitch  of  the 
resonance  of  some  fine  old  violins  bv  Duiflo- 
pmgoar  (Swiss  Tyrol*  Bologna,  and  Lyons 
1510-1530)*  Amati  (Cremona  1596-1684),  Bug- 
gieri  (Cremona  1608- 1720),  Stradivari  (Cre- 
mona 1644^1737),  Giuseppe  Gameri  (known  as 
'Joseph,'  Cremona  16S3-1745),  Lnpot  (France 
i750-i82O>.  The  method  adopted  was  to  hold 
timing-forks,  of  which  the  exact  pitch  had 
been  determined  by  Scheibler's  forks,  in  saoces- 
sion  over  the  widest  part  of  the/  hole  on  the 
^  string  side  of  the  violin  (furthest  from  the 
somid-post)  and  observe  what  fork  excited  the 
maiimnm  resonance.  My  forks  form  a  series 
proceeding  by  4  vib.  in  a  second,  and  hence  I 
oould  only  tell  the  pitch  within  2  vib.,  and  it 
was  often  extremely  difficult  to  decide  on  the 
fork  which  gave  the  best  resonance.  By  far 
the  strongest  resonance  lay  between  268  and 
272  vib.»  but  one  early  Stradivari  (1696)  had  a 
fine  resonance  at  264  vib.  There  was  also  a 
secondary  but  weaker  maximum  resonance  at 
about  252  vib.  The  256  vib.  was  generally 
decidedly  inferior.  Hence  we  may  take  270 
vib.  as  the  primary  maximum,  and  252  vib.  as 
the  secondary.  The  first  corresponds  to  the 
highest  English  concert  pitch  c''«540  vib., 
now  used  in  London,  and  agrees  with  the 
lower  resonance  of  Bausch's  instrument  men- 
tioned in  the  text.  The  second,  which  is  120 
cents,  or  rather  more  than  an  equal  Semitone 
flatter,  gives  the  pitch  which  my  researches 
shew  was  common  over  all  Europe  at  the 
time  (see  App.  XX.  sect.  H.).  But  although 
ihe  low  pitch  was  prevalent,  a  high  pitch,  a 
great  Semitone  (117  ct.)  hi^er,  was  also  in 
use  as  a  *  ohambcff  pitch.*  A  violin  of  Mazzini 
of  Brescia  (1560.1640)  belonging  to  the  eldest 
dau^ter  of  Mr.  Yemon  Lushington,  Q.C.,  had 
the  same  two  maximum  resonances,  the  higher 
being  decidedly  the  superior.    I  did  not  ex- 


amine for  the  higher  or  a*  pitches  named  in 
the  text.  Mr.  Healey  (of  the  Science  and  Art 
Department,  South  Kensington)  thought  his 
violin  ^supposed  to  be  an  Amati)  sounded  best 
at  the  low  pitch  c"  »  504.  Subsequently,  I  ex- 
amined a  fine  instrument,  bearing  inside  it  the 
label  *  Petrus  Guamerius  Cremonensis  fecit, 
MantuBB  sub  titulo  S.Theresiie,  anno  1701,*  in  ^ 
the  possession  of  Mr.  A.  J.  Hipkins,  who  knew  ■< 
it  to  be  genuine.  I  tried  this  with  a  series 
of  forks,  proceeding  by  differences  of  about 
4  vibrations  from  240  to  56a  It  was  surprising 
to  find  that  every  fork  was  to  a  certain  extent 
reinforced,  that  is,  in  no  case  was  the  tone 
quenched,  and  in  no  case  was  it  reduced  in 
strength.  But  at  260  vib.  there  was  a  good, 
and  at  364  a  better  resonance;  perhaps  262 
may  therefore  be  taken  as  the  best.  There 
was  no  secondary  low  resonance,  but  there 
were  two  higher  resonances,  one  about  472, 
(although  468  and  476  were  also  good,)  and 
another  at  520  (although  524  and  528  were 
also  good).  As  this  sheet  was  passing  through 
the  press  I  had  an  opportunity  of  trying  the 
resonance  of  Dr.  Huggins's  Stradivari  of  1708, 
figured  in  Grovels  Dictionary  of  MusiCt  iii. 
728,  as  a  specimen  of  the  best  period  of  Stradi-  « 
vari's  work.  The  result  was  essentially  the 
same  as  the  last ;  every  fork  was  more  or  less 
reinforced ;  there  was  a  subordinate  maximum 
at  252  vib. ;  a  better  at  from  260  to  268  vib. ; 
very  slight  maxima  at  312,  348,  384,  412,  420, 
428  (the  last  of  which  was  the  best,  but  was 
only  a  fair  reinforcement),  472  to  480,  but  520 
was  decidedly  best,  and  540  good.  No  one 
fork  was  reinforced  to  the  extent  it  would  have 
been  on  a  resonator  properly  tuned  to  it,  but 
no  one  note  was  deteriorated.  Dr.  Huggins  says 
that '  the  strong  feature  of  this  violin  is  the 
great  equality  of  all  four  strings  and  the  per- 
sistenoe  of  the  same  fine  quality  of  tone 
throughout  the  entire  range  of  the  instru- 
ment.' —  Tianslator.] 


Digitized  by 


Google 


83 


MUSICAL  TONES  OP  FLUTE  OR  FLUE  PIPES.        pabt  i. 


Fig.  27. 


/a 


I 


them  are  a  Twelfth  lower  than  on  the  violin.  On  the  other  hand,  the  most 
favoured  partial  tones  of  the  vi61a  (tenor)  corresponding  nearly  with  h\  do  not 
lie  between  the  first  and  second  strings,  but 
between  the  second  and  third ;  and  this  seems 
to  be  connected  with  the  altered  quality  of 
tone  on  this  instrument.  Unfortunately  this 
influence  cannot  be  expressed  numerically. 
The  maximum  of  resonance  for  the  proper 
tones  of  the  body  of  air  is  not  very  marked ; 
were  it  otherwise  there  would  be  much  more 
inequaHty  in  the  scale  as  played  on  these 
bowed  instruments,  inmiediately  on  passing 
the  pitch  of  the  proper  tones  of  their  bodies  of 
f  air.  We  must  consequently  conjecture  that 
their  influence  upon  the  relative  intensity  of 
the  individual  partials  in  the  musical  tones  of 
these  instruments  is  not  very  prominent. 

5.  Musical  Tones  of  Flute  or  Flue  Pipes. 

In  these  instruments  the  tone  is  produced 

by  driving  a  stream  of  air  against  an  opening, 

generally  furnished  with  sharp  edges,  in  some 
\    hollow  space  filled  with  air.     To  this  class 

belong  the  bottles  described  in  the  last  chapter, 

and  shewn  in  fig.  20  (p.  60c),  and  especially 

flutes  and  the  majority  of  organ  pipes.     For 

flutes,  the  resonant  body  of  air  is  included  in 
H  its  own  cylindrical  bore.    It  is  blown  with  the 

mouth,  which  directs  the  breath  against  the 

somewhat  sharpened  edges  of  its  mouth  hole. 

The  construction  of  organ  pipes  will  be  seen 

from  the  two  adjacent  figures.     Fig.  27,  A, 

shews  a  square  wooden  pipe,  cut  open  long- 
wise, and  B  the  external  appearance  of  a  round 

tin  pipe.    B  B  in  each  shews  the  tube  which 

incloses  the  sonorous  body  of  air,  a  b  is  the 

mouth  where  it  is  blown,  terminating  in  a  sharp 

lip.    In  fig.  27,  A,  we  see  the  air  chamber  or 

throat  E  into  which  the  air  is  first  driven  from 

the  bellows,  and  whence  it  can  only  escape 

through  the  narrow  slit  c  d,  which  directs  it 
H  against  the  edge  of  the  Hp.     The  wooden  pipe 

A  as  here  drawn  is  open,  that  is  its  extremity 

is  uncovered,  and  it  produces  a  tone  with  a 

wave  of  air  tivice  as  long  as  the  tube  B  B. 

The  other  pipe,  B,  is  stopped,  that  is,  its  upper 

extremity  is  closed.    Its  tone  has  a  vrmefour 

times  the  length  of  the  tube  B  B,  and  hence  an 

Octave  deeper  than  an  open  pipe  of  the  same 

length.* 

Any  air  chambers  can  be  made  to  give  a 

musical  tone,  just  like  organ  pipes,  flutes,  the  bottles  previously  described,  ihe 

windchests  of  vioUns,  Ac,  provided  they  have  a  sufticiently  narrow  opening, 


0 


*  [These  relations  are  only  approximate, 
as  is  explained  below.  The  mode  of  excite- 
ment by  the  lip  of  the  pipe  makes  them 


inexact.      Also  they   take  no  notice  of  the 
*  scale  *  or  diameters  and  depths  of  the  pipes, 
or  of  the  force  ol  the  wind,  or  of  the  tempera- 
Digitized  by  V^ O OQ IC 


CHAP.  V.  5.    MUSICAL  TONES  OP  FLUTE  OB  FLUE  PIPES. 


89 


famished  with  somewhat  projectmg  sharp  edges,  by  directing  a  thin  flat  stream  of 
air  across  the  opening  and  against  its  edges.* 

The  motion  of  air  that  takes  place  in  the  inside  of  organ  pipes,  corresponds  to 
a  system  of  plane  waves  which  are  reflected  backwards  and  forwards  between  the  ^ 
two  ends  of  the  pipe.  At  the  stopped  end  of  a  cylindrical  pipe  the  reflexion  of 
every  wave  that  strikes  it  is  very  perfect,  so  that  the  reflected  wave  has  the  same 
intensity  as  it  had  before  reflexion.  In  any  train  of  waves  moving  in  a  given 
direction,  the  velocity  of  the  oscillating  molecules  in  the  condensed  portion  of  the 
wave  takes  place  in  ihe  same  direction  as  that  of  the  propagation  of  the  waves,  and 
in  the  rarefied  portion  in  the  opposite  direction.  But  at  the  stopped  end  of  a  pipe 
its  cover  does  not  allow  of  any  forward  motion  of  the  molecules  of  air  in  the 
direction  of  the  length  of  the  pipe.  Hence  the  incident  and  reflected  wave  at  this 
place  combine  so  as  to  excite  opposite  velocities  of  oscillation  of  the  molecules  of 
air,  and  consequently  by  their  superposition  the  velocity  of  the  molecules  of  air  at  ^ 
the  cover  is  destroyed.  Hence  it  follows  that  the  phases  of  pressure  in  both  will 
agree,  because  opposite  motions  of  oscillation  and  opposite  propagation,  result  in 
accordant  pressure. 

Hence  at  the  stopped  end  there  is  no  motion,  but  great  alteration  of  pressure. 
The  reflexion  of  the  wave  takes  place  in  such  a  manner  that  the  phase  of  conden* 
sation  remains  unaltered,  but  the  direction  of  the  motion  of  oscillation  is  reversed. 

The  contrary  takes  place  at  the  open  end  of  pipes,  in  which  is  also  included  the 
opening  of  their  mouths.  At  an  open  end  where  the  air  of  the  pipe  communi- 
cates freely  with  the  great  outer  mass  of  air,  no  sensible  condensation  can  take 
place.  In  the  explanation  usually  given  of  the  motion  of  air  in  pipes,  it  is  assumed 
that  both  condensation  and  rarefEMstion  vanish  at  the  open  ends  of  pipes,  which  is 
approximatively  but  not  exactly  correct.  If  there  were  exactly  no  alteration  of 
density  at  that  place,  there  would  be  complete  reflexion  of  every  incident  wave 
at  the  open  ends,  so  that  an  equally  large  reflected  wave  would  be  generated  with  ^ 
an  opposite  state  of  density,  but  the  direction  of  oscillation  of  the  molecules  of 
air  in  both  waves  would  agree.    The  superposition  of  such  an  incident  and  such  a 


tore  of  the  air.  The  following  are  adapted 
from  the  roles  given  by  M.  GavaU16-Goll,  the 
celebrated  Frendi  oigan-builder,  in  Comptea 
Rwdu9t  i860,  p.  176,  supposing  the  tempera- 
ture to  be  59*  F.  or  15**  C,  and  the  pressure  of 
the  wind  to  be  about  3^  inohes,  or  8  centi- 
metres (meaning  that  it  will  support  a  column 
of  water  of  that  height  in  the  wind  gauge). 
The  pitch  numbers,  for  double  vibrations,  are 
found  by  dividing  20,080  when  the  dimensions 
are  given  in  inches,  and  510,000  when  in 
millimetres  by  the  following  numbers :  (i)  for 
cylindrical  open  pipes,  3  times  the  length 
added  to  5  times  the  diameter ;  (2)  for  cyUndri- 
cal  slopped  pipes,  6  times  the  lexigth  added  to 
10  times  the  diameter;  (3)  for  square  open 
pipcSf  3  times  the  length  added  to  6  times  the 
depth  (clear  internal  distance  from  mouth  to 
back) ;  {4)  for  square  stopped  pipes,  6  times  the 
length  added  to  12  times  the  depth. 

This  rule  is  always  sufficiently  accurate  for 
cutting  organ  pipes  to  their  approximate 
length,  and  piercing  them  to  bring  out  the 
Octave  harmonic,  and  has  long  been  used  for 
these  purposes  in  M.  Cavaill6-Ck>irB  factory. 
The  rule  is  not  so  safe  for  the  square  wooden 
as  for  the  cylindrical  metal  pipes.  The  pitch 
of  a  pipe  of  known  dimensions  ought  to  be 
first  ascertained  by  other  means.  Then  this 
pitch  number  multiplied  by  the  divisors  in  (3) 
and  (4)  should  be  used  in  place  of  the  20,080 
or  510,000  of  the  rule,  for  all  similar  pipes. 

As  to  strength  of  wind,  as  pressure  varies 


from  2}  to  3^  inches,  the  pitch  number 
increases  by  about  1  in  300,  but  as  pressure 
varies  from  3i  to  4  inches,  the  pitch  number 
increases  by  about  i  in  440,  the  whole  increase 
of  pressure  from  2|  to  4  inches  increases  the 
pitch  number  by  i  m  180. 

Por  temperature,  I  found  by  numerous 
observations  at  very  different  temperatures' 
that  the  following  practical  rule  is  sufficient 
for  reducing  the  pitch  number  observed  at  one 
temperature  to  that  due  to  another.  It  is  not 
quite  accurate,  for  the  air  blown  from  the 
bellows  is  often  lower  than  the  external  tem- 
perature. Let  P  be  the  pitch  number  observed  f 
at  a  given  temperature,  and  d  the  difference  of 
temperature  in  degrees  Fahr.  Then  the  pitch 
number  is  P  x  ^i  ±  '00104CQ  according  as  the 
temperature  is  higher  or  lower.  The  practical 
operation  is  as  follows :  supposing  P  »  528,  and 
(2=14  increase  of  temperature.  To  528  add 
4  in  100,  or  21*12,  giving  549*12,  Divide  by 
1000  to  2  places  of  decimals,  giving  *55.. 
Multiply  hj  d-  14,  giving  7*70.  Adding  this  to 
528,  we  get  535*7  for  the  pitch  number  at  the 
new  temperature. — Translator,] 

*  [Here  the  passage  from  'These  edges,' 
p.  140,  to  *  resembling  a  violin,*  p.  141  of  the 
1st  English  edition,  has  been  omitted,  and  the 
passage  from  *  The  motion  of  air,'  p.  89a, 
to  *  their  comers  are  rounded  off,'  p.  936,  haa 
been  inserted  in  accordance  with  the  4th 
Grerman  edition. — Translator.] 


Digitized  by  VjOOQIC 


90  MUSICAL  TONES  OP  FLUTE  OR  FLUE  PIPES.        pabt  i. 

reflected  wave  would  indeed  leave  the  state  of  density  unaltered  at  the  open  ends, 
but  would  occasion  great  velocity  in  the  oscillating  molecules  of  air. 

In  reality  the  assumption  made  explains  the  essential  phenomena  of  organ  pipes. 
Consider  first  a  pipe  with  two  open  ends.  On  our  exciting  a  wave  of  condensation 
at  one  end,  it  runs  forward  to  the  other  end,  is  there  reflected  as  a  wave  of  rare- 
faction, runs  back  to  the  first  end,  is  here  again  reflected  with  another  alteration  of 
phase,  as  a  wave  of  condensation,  and  then  repeats  the  same  path  in  the  same  way 
a  second  time.  This  repetition  of  the  same  process  therefore  occurs,  after  the 
wave  in  the  tube  has  passed  once  forwards  and  once  backwards,  that  is  twice  through 
the  whole  length  of  the  tube.  The  time  required  to  do  this  is  equal  to  double  the 
length  of  the  pipe  divided  by  the  velocity  of  sound.  This  is  the  duration  of  the 
vibration  of  the  deepest  tone  which  the  pipe  can  give. 

Suppose  now  that  at  the  time  when  the  wave  begins  its  second  forward  and 

f  backward  journey,  a  second  impulse  in  the  same  direction  is  given,  say  by  a  vibra- 
ting tuning-fork.  The  motion  of  the  air  wiU  then  receive  a  reinforcement,  which 
will  constantly  increase,  if  the  fresh  impulses  take  place  in  the  same  rhythm  as  the 
forward  and  backward  progression  of  the  waves. 

Even  if  the  returning  wave  does  not  coincide  with  the  first  following  similar 
impulse  of  the  tuniag-fork,  but  only  with  the  second  or  third  or  fourth  and  so  on, 
the  motion  of  the  air  wiU  be  reinforced  after  every  forward  and  backward  passage. 
A  tube  open  at  both  ends  will  therefore  serve  as  a  resonator  for  tones  whose 
pitch  number  is  equal  to  the  velocity  of  sound  (332  metres)  *  divided  by  twice  the 
length  of  the  tube,  or  some  multiple  of  that  number.  That  is  to  say,  the  tones  of 
strongest  resonance  for  such  a  tube  wiU,  as  in  strings,  form  the  complete  series  of 
harmonic  upper  partials  of  its  prime. 

The  case  is  somewhat  different  for  pipes  stopped  at  one  end.  If  at  the  open 
end,  by  means  of  a  vibrating  tuning-fork,  we  excite  an  impulse  of  condensation 

%  which  propagates  itself  along  the  tube,  it  will  run  on  to  the  stopped  end,  will  be 
there  reflected  as  a  wave  of  condensation,  return,  will  be  again  reflected  at  the 
open  end  with  altered  phase  as  a  wave  of  rarefaction,  and  only  after  it  has  been 
again  reflected  at  the  stopped  end  with  a  similar  phase,  and  then  once  more  at  the 
open  end  with  an  altered  phase  as  a  condensation,  will  a  repetition  of  the  process 
ensue,  that  is  to  say,  not  till  after  it  has  traversed  the  length  of  the  pipe  four  times. 
Hence  the  prime  tone  of  a  stopped  pipe  has  twice  as  long  a  period  of  vibration  as  an 

I  open  pipe  of  the  same  length.  That  is  to  say,  the  stopped  pipe  wiU  be  an  Octave 
deeper  than  the  open  pipe.  If,  then,  after  this  double  forward  and  backward  passage, 
the  first  impulse  is  renewed,  there  will  arise  a  reinforcement  of  resonance. 

Partials  f  of  the  prime  tone  can  also  be  reinforced,  but  only  those  which  are 
unevenly  numbered.  For  since  at  the  expiration  of  half  the  period  of  vibration, 
the  prime  tone  of  the  wave  in  the  tube  renews  its  path  with  an  opposite  phase  of 
density,  only  such  tones  can  be  reinforced  as  have  an  opposite  phase  at  the  expira- 

^  tion  of  half  the  period  of  vibration.  But  at  this  time  the  second  partial  has  just 
completed  a  whole  vibration,  the  fourth  partial  two  whole  vibrations,  and  so  on. 

*  [This  is  1089-3  '^t  in  a  second,  which  before  the  Physical    Society,  and  published 

is  the  mean  of  several  observations  in  free  in  the  Philosophical  Moffoeine  tor  Dec.  1883, 

air ;  it  is  usual,  however,  in  England  to  take  pp.  447-455,  and  Oct.  1884,  pp.  328-334,  as  the 

the  whole  number  1090  feet,  at  freezing.    At  means  of  many  observations  on  the  velocity 

60°  F.  it  is  about  1 120  feet  per  second.    Mr.  D.  of  sound  in  dry  air  at  32^  F.,  in  tubes,  obtained 
J.  Blaikley  (see  note  p.  gjd),  in  two  papers  read 


for  diameter  '45 

pitch  various,  velocity     1064*26 
pitch  260  vib. ,  velocity    1 062'  1 2 


•75 
107253 
1072-47 


1-25 

1078-71 
1078-73 


2-08 
1081-78 
1082-51 


3*47  English  inches. 
1083-13       „        feet. 
1084-88      „ 


The  velocity  in  tubes  is  therefore  always  less  note  p.  23c,)  but  it  is  precisely  the  latter  which 

than  in  free  hix,— Translator,]  are  not  excited  in  the  present  case.    This  is 

t  [The    original    says    *  upper    partials'  only  mentioned  as  a  warning  to  those  who 

(Obert&ne),  but  the  upper  partials  which  are  faultily  use  the  faulty  expression  *  overtones  * 

unevenly  numbered  are  the  ist,  3rd,  5th,  &c.»  indifferently    for    both    partials    and    upper 

and  these  are  really  the  2nd,  4th,  6th,  &c.,  (that  partials.— ZVa9w2a(or.] 
is,  the  evenly  numbered)  partial  tones,  (see  foot- 


Digitized  by 


Google 


cHAP.v.s.    MUSICAL  TONES  OF  FLUTE  OR  FLUE  PIPES.  91 

These  therefore  have  the  same  phases,  and  cancel  their  effect  on  the  return  of  the 
wave  with  an  opposite  phase.  Hence  the  tones  of  strongest  resonance  in  stopped 
pipes  correspond  with  the  series  of  unevenly  numbered  partials  of  its  fundamental 
tone.  Supposing  its  pitch  number  is  n,  then  ^n  is  the  Twelfth  of  n,  that  is  the 
Fifth  of  2n  the  higher  Octave,  and  ^n  is  the  major  Third  of  4n  the  next  higher 
Octave,  and  jn  the  [sub]  minor  Seventh  of  the  same  Octave,  and  so  on. 

Now  although  the  phenomena  follow  these  rules  in  the  principal  points,  certain 
deviations  from  them  occur  because  there  is  not  precisely  no  change  of  pressure 
at  the  open  ends  of  pipes.  From  these  ends  the  motion  of  sound  communicates 
itself  to  the  unindosed  air  beyond,  and  the  waves  which  spread  out  from  the  open 
ends  of  the  tubes  have  relatively  very  little  alteration  of  pressure,  but  are  not 
entirely  without  some.  Hence  a  part  of  every  wave  which  is  incident  on  the  open 
end  of  the  pipe  is  not  reflected,  but  runs  out  into  the  open  air,  while  the  remainder 
or  greater  portion  of  the  wave  is  reflected,  and  returns  into  the  tube.  The  re-  % 
flexion  is  the  more  complete,  the  smaller  are  the  dimensions  of  the  opening  of  \\ 
the  tube  in  comparison  with  the  wave-length  of  the  tone  in  question. 

Theory*  also,  agreeing  with  experiment,  shews  that  the  phases  of  the  reflected 
part  of  the  wave  are  the  same  as  they  would  be  if  the  reflexion  did  not  take  place 
at  the  siirfB.ce  of  the  opening  itself  but  at  another  and  somewhat  different  plane. 
H^ice  what  may  be  called  the  redticed  length  of  the  pipe,  or  that  answering  to  the 
pitch,  is  somewhat  different  from  the  real  length,  and  the  difference  between  the 
two  depends  on  the  form  of  the  mouth,  and  not  on  the  pitch  of  the  notes  pro- 
duced unless  they  are  so  high  and  hence  their  wave-lengths  so  short,  that  the 
dimensions  of  the  opening  cannot  be  neglected  in  respect  to  them. 

For  cylindrical  pipes  of  circular  section,  with  ends  cut  at  right  angles  to  the 
length,  the  distance  of  the  plane  of  reflexion  from  the  end  of  the  pipe  is  theoreti- 
cally determined  to  be  at  a  distance  of  0*7854  the  radius  of  the  circle.f    For  a 
wooden  pipe  of  square  section,  of  which  the  sides  were  36  mm.  (1*4  inch)  internal  11 
measure,  I  found  the  distance  of  the  plane  of  reflexion  14  mm.  (-55  inch).j: 

Now  since  on  account  of  the  imperfect  reflexion  of  waves  at  the  open  ends  of 
organ  pipes  (and  respectively  at  their  mouths)  a  part  of  the  motion  of  the  air  must 
escape  into  i^e  free  air  at  every  vibration,  any  oscillatory  motion  of  its  mass  of  air 
must  be  speedily  exhausted,  if  there  are  no  forces  to  replace  the  lost  motion.  In  . 
fact,  on  ceasing  to  blow  an  organ  pipe  scarcely  any  after  sound  is  observable.  I 
Nevertheless  the  wave  is  frequently  enough  reflected  forward  and  backward  for  its 
pitch  to  become  perceptible  on  tapping  against  the  pipe. 

The  means  usuaUy  adopted  for  keeping  them  continually  sounding,  is  blowing. 
In  order  to  understand  the  action  of  this  process,  we  must  remember  that  when 

*  See  my  paper  in  CreUe's  Journal  for  tion  of  the  plug.     [The  Bameness  of  the  pitch 

MathemaHcSt  vol.  Ivii.  ia  detennined  by  seeing  that  each  makes  the 

t  Mr.  Bosanqaet  (Proe.  Mtta.  Aasn.  1877-8,  same  number  of  beats  with  the  same  fork.] 

p.  65)  is  reported  as  saying:*  Lord  Bay  leigh  and  The  nodal  sorfaoe  lay  137  mm.  (5-39  inoh) 

himself  had  gone  fully  into  the  matter,  and  had  from  the  end  of  the  pipe,  while  a  quarter  of  1l 

oome  to  the  condusion  that  this  correction  was  a  wave-length  was  151  mm.  (5*94  inoh).    At  the 

much  less  than  Helmholtz  supposed.  Lord  Bay-  mouth  end  of  the  pipe,  on  tiie  other  hand, 

leigh  adopted  the  figure  *6  of  the  radius,  whilst  83  mm.  (3*27  inoh)  jrere  wanting  to  complete 

he  himself  adopted  -55.'    See  papers  by  Lord  the  theoretical  length  of  the  pipe.    [The  addi- 

Rayleigh  and  Mr.  Bosanquet  in  PhUoaophical  tional  piece  being  half  the  length  of  the  wave, 

MoMmne.    Mr.   Blaikley  by  a  new   process  the  pitdi  of  the  pipe  before  and  after  the 

finds  *576,  which  lies  between  the  other  two,  addition  of  this  piece  remains  the  same,  by 

see  hispaper  in  Phil.  Miig.  May  1879,  p.  342.  which  property  the  length  of  the  additions 

X  The  pipe  was  of  wood,  made  by  Marloye,  piece  is  found.    The  length  of  the  pipe  from 

the  additional  length  being  302  mm.  (11*9  in.),  the  bottom  of  the  mouth  to  the  open  end  was 

corresponding  exactly  with  half  the  length  of  205  mm.  =  8-07  inch ;  the  node,  as  determined,, 

wave  of  the  pipe.    The  position  of  the  nodal  was  137  nun. » 5*39  inch  from  the  open  end, 

plane  in  the  inside  of  the  pipe  was  determined  and  68  mm. «  2*68  inch  from  the  bottom  of  the 


by  inserting  a  wooden  plug  ox  the  same  diameter      mouth.    These  lengths  had  to  be  increased  by 
•s  of  the  mtemal  opening  of  the  pipe  at  its      14  mm. » *55  in.  and  83  mm. » 3*27  in.  respeo- 

quarter  length 
— 2Van«fcUor.] 


pipe  .              __                _             _                _ 

open  end,  until  the  pitch  of  the  pipe,  which  tively,  to  xnake  up  each  to  the  quarter  length 

nad  now  beoome  a  closed  one,  was  exactly  the  of  the  wave  151  mm. »  5*95  inch.~2Van«2ator.] 
nme  as  that  of  the  open  pipe  before  the  inser- 


Digitized  by^ 


93  MUSICAL  TONES  OP  FLUTE  OB  FLUE  PIPES.        pabt  i.. 

air  is  blown  out  of  snch  a  slit  as  that  which  lies  below  the  lip  of  the  pipe,  it  breaks 
through  the  air  which  lies  at  rest  in  &ont  of  the  slit  in  a  thin  sheet  like  a  blade  or 
lamina,  and  hence  at  £rst  does  not  draw  any  sensible  part  of  that  air  into  its  own 
motion.  It  is  not  tiU  it  reaches  a  distance  of  some  centimetres  [a  centimetre  is 
nearly  four-tenths  of  an  inch]  that  the  outpouring  sheet  splits  up  into  eddies  or 
vortices,  which  effect  a  mixture  of  the  air  at  rest  and  the  air  in  motion.  This 
blade-shaped  sheet  of  air  in  motion  can  be  rendered  visible  by  sending  a  stream  of 
air  impregnated  with  smoke  or  clouds  of  salammoniac  through  the  mouth  of  a 
pipe  from  which  the  pipe  itself  is  removed,  such  as  is  commonly  found  among 
physical  apparatus.  Any  blade-shaped  gas  flame  which  comes  from  a  split  burner 
is  also  an  example  of  a  similar  process.  Burning  renders  visible  the  limits  between 
the  outpouring  sheet  of  gas  and  the  atmosphere.  But  the  flame  does  not  render 
the  continuation  of  the  stream  visible. 
IF  Now  the  blade-shaped  sheet  of  air  at  the  mouth  of  the  organ  pipe  is  wafiied  to 
one  side  or  the  other  by  every  stream  of  air  which  touches  its  surface,  exactly  as 
this  gas  flame  is.  The  consequence  is  that  when  the  oscillation  of  the  mass  of  air 
in  the  pipe  *  causes  the  air  to  enter  through  the  ends  of  the  pipe,  the  blade-shaped 
stream  of  air  arising  from  the  mouth  is  also  inclined  inwards,  and  drives  its  whole 
mass  of  air  into  the  pipe.f  During  the  opposite  phase  of  vibration,  on  the  other 
hand,  when  the  air  leaves  the  ends  of  the  pipe  the  whole  mass  of  this  blade  of  air 
is  driven  outwards.  Hence  it  happens  that  exactly  at  the  times  when  the  air  in 
^  the  pipe  is  most  condensed,  more  air  still  is  driven  in  from  the  bellows,  whence 
the  condensation,  and  consequently  also  the  equivalent  of  work  of  the  vibration  of 
the  air  is  increased,  while  at  the  periods  of  rarefaction  in  the  pipe  the  wind  of  the 
bellows  pours  its  mass  of  air  into  the  open  space  in  front  of  the  pipe.  We  must 
remember  also  that  the  blade-shaped  sheet  of  air  requires  time  in  order  to  traverse 
the  width  of  the  mouth  of  the  pipe,  and  is  during  this  time  exposed  to  the  action . 
If  of  the  vibrating  column  of  air  in  the  pipe,  and  does  not  reach  the  lip  (that  is  the 
line  where  the  two  paths,  inwards  and  outwards,  intersect)  until  the  end  of  this 
time.  Every  particle  of  air  that  is  blown  in,  consequently  reaches  a  phase  of 
vibration  in  the  interior  of  the  pipe,  which  is  somewhat  later  than  that  to  which 
it  was  exposed  ui  traversing  the  opening.  If  the  latter  motion  was  inwards,  it 
encounters  the  following  condensation  in  the  interior  of  the  pipe,  and  so  on. 

This  mode  of  exciting  the  tone  conditions  also  the  peculiar  quality  of  tone  of 
these  organ  pipes.  We  may  regard  the  blade-shaped  stream  of  air  as  very  thin  in 
comparison  with  the  amplitude  of  the  vibrations  of  air.  The  latter  often  amount 
to  lo  or  i6  millimetres  ('39  to  '63  inches),  as  may  be  seen  by  bringing  small 
flames  of  gas  close  to  this  opemng.  Gonse.quently  the  alternation  between  the 
periods  of  time  for  which  the  whole  blast  is  poured  iato  the  interior  of  the  pipe, 
and  those  for  which  it  is  entirely  emptied  outside,  is  rather  sudden,  in  fact  almost 
instantaneous.  Hence  it  follows  %  that  the  oscillations  excited  by  blowing  are  of 
^  a  similar  kind ;  namely,  that  for  a  certain  part  of  each  vibration  the  velocity  of  the 
particles  of  air  in  the  mouth  and  in  free  space,  have  a  constant  value  directed  out- 
wards, and  for  a  second  portion  of  the  same,  a  constant  value  directed  inwards. 
With  stronger  blowing  that  directed  inwards  will  be  more  intense  and  of  shorter 
duration ;  with  weaker  blowing,  the  converse  may  take  place.  Moreover,  the  pres- 
sure in  the  mass  of  air  put  in  motion  in  the  pipe  must  also  alternate  between  two 
constant  values  with  considerable  rapidity.  The  rapidity  of  this  change  will, 
however,  be  moderated  by  the  circumstance  thai  the  blade-shaped  sheet  of  air  is 
not  infinitely  thin,  but  requires  a  short  time  to  pass  over  the  lip  of  the  pipe,  and 

*  [It  has,  however,  not  been  explained  how  side  the  pipe  is  very  small.    A  candle  flame 

that  *  oscillation '  commences.    This  will  be  held  at  the  end  of  the  pipe  only  pulsates ; 

alluded  to  in  the  additions  to  App.  VII.  sect.  B.  held  a  few  inches  from  the  lip,  along  the  edge 

— Translator.]  of  the  pipe,  it  is  speedily  extinguished.— Trans- 

t  [The  amount  of  air  which  enters  as  com-  latorJ] 
pared  with  that  which  passes  ov«r  the  lip  out-  %  ^^  Appendix  VII.  [especially  sect.  B,  II.]. 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


CHAP.  V.  5.    MUSICAL  TONES  OP  FLUTE  OR  FLUE  PIPES.  93 

that  secondly  the  higher  upper  partials,  whose  wave-lengths  only  slightly  exceed  the 
diameter  of  the  pipe,  are  as  a  general  rule  imperfectly  developed. 

The  kind  of  motion  of  the  air  here  described  is  exactly  the  same  as  that  shewn 
in  fig.  23  (p.  826),  B  and  0,  fig.  24  (p.  836),  A  and  B,  for  the  vibrating  points  of   i 
a  violin  string.    Organ-builders  have  long  since  remarked  the  similarity  of  the 
quality  of  tone,  for  the  narrower  cylindrical-pipe  stops  when  strongly  blown,  as 
shewn  by  the  names  :  Geigenprincipal,  Vidla  di  Gamba,  Violoncello,  Violon-bcLSS.* 

That  these  conclusions  firom  the  mechanics  of  blowing  correspond  with  the 
£Gu;ts  in  nature,  is  shewn  by  the  experiments  of  Messrs.  Toepler  &  Boltzmann,t  who 
rendered  the  form  of  the  oscillation  of  pressure  in  the  interior  of  the  pipe  optically 
observable  by  the  interference  of  light  passed  through  a  node  of  the  vibrating  mass 
of  air.  When  the  force  of  the  wind  was  small  they  found  almost  a  simple  vibration 
(the  smaller  the  oscillation  of  the  air-blade  at  the  lip,  the  more  completely  the  dis- 
continuities disappear).  But  when  the  force  of  the  wind  was  greater  they  found  ^ 
a  very  rapid  alternation  between  two  different  values  of  pressure,  each  of  which 
remained  almost  unaltered  for  a  fraction  of  a  vibration. 

Messrs.  Mach  and  J.  Hervert's  X  experiments  with  gas  flames  placed  before  the 
end  of  an  open  pipe  to  make  the  vibrations  visible,  shew  that  the  form  of  motion 
just  described  really  occurs  at  the  ends  of  the  pipes.  The  forms  of  vibration  which 
they  deduced  from  the  analysis  of  the  forms  of  the  flames  correspond  with  those  of 
a  violin  string,  except  that,  for  the  reason  given  above,  their  comers  are  rounded  off. 

By  using  resonators  I  find  that  on  narrow  pipes  of  this  kind  the  partial  tones  V 
may  be  clearly  heard  up  to  the  sixth. 

For  wide  open  pipes,  on  the  other  hand,  the  adjacent  proper  tones  of  the  tube 
are  all  somewhat  sharper  than  the  corresponding  harmonic  tones  of  the  prime,  and 
hence  these  tones  will  be  much  less  reinforced  by  the  resonance  of  the  tube.  Wide 
pipes,  having  larger  masses  of  vibrating  air  and  admitting  of  being  much  more 
strongly  blown  without  jumping  up  into  an  harmonic,  are  used  for  the  great  body  i[ 
of  sound  on  the  organ,  and  are  hence  called  prificipalstimfnen,^  For  the  above 
reasons  they  produce  the  prime  tone  alone  strongly  and  fully,  with  a  much  weaker 
retinue  of  secondary  tones.  For  wooden  '  principal '  pipes,  I  find  the  prime  tone 
lufid  its  Octave  or  first  upper  partial  very  distinct;  the  Twelfth  or  second  upper 
partial  is  but  weak,  and  the  higher  upper  partials  no  longer  distinctly  perceptible. 
For  metal  pipes  the  fourth  partial  was  also  still  perceptible.  The  quality  of  tone  in 
these  pipes  is  faller  and  softer  than  that  of  the  geigenprincipaL*  When  flute  or 
flue  stops  of  the  organ,  and  the  German  flute  are  blown  softly,  the  upper  partials 
lose  strength  at  a  greater  rate  than  the  prime  tone,  and  hence  the  musical  quality 
becomes  weak  and  soft. 

Another  variety  is  observed  on  the  pipes  which  are  conically  narrowed  at  their 

*  [Oetgenprincipal — violin  or   crisp-toned  eively  conical  with  a  bell  top.    Prom  Hopkins 

diapason,  8  feet, — violin  principal,  4  feet.    See  on  the  Organ,  pp.  137,  445,  (fee. — Translator.] 
supra  p.  gid,  note.    Violoncello — *  crisp-toned  f  Poggendorff^s  Annd.,  vol.  czli.  pp.  321-  ^ 

open  stop,  of  small  scale,  the  Octave  to  the  352. 

violone,  8    feet.'     Violon-baas— ihis    fails   in  :(  Poggendor£f*s  ^nnaZ.,  vol.  cxlvii.  pp.  590- 

Hopkins,  but   it  is  probably  his  *violone —  604. 

doable  bass,  a  unison  open  wood  stop,  of  mnoh  §  [Literally  *  principal  voices  or  parts ; ' 

smaller  scale  than  the  Diapason,  and  formed  may  probably  be  best  translated  '  principal 

of  pipes  that  are  a  little  wider  at  the  top  than  work  *  or  *  diapason-work,'  including  *  all  the 

at  the  bottom,  and  famished  with  ears  and  open    cylindrical    stops    of   Open    Diapason 

beard  at  the  mouth ;  the  tone  of  the  Violone  measure,  or  which  have  their  scale  deduced 

is  crisp  and  resonant,  like  that  of  the  orches-  from  that  of  the  Open  Diapason ;  such  stops 

tral  Double  Bass ;  and  its  speech  being  a  little  are  the  chief,  most  important  or  ** principal," 

slow,  it  has  the  Stopped  Bass  always  drawn  as  they  are  also  most  numerous  in  an  organ, 

with  it,  16  feet.'     Qamba  or  viol  da  ganiba —  The    Unison    and    Double  Open  Diapasons, 

*  bass  vioL  unison  stop,  of  smaller  scale,  and  Principal,  Fifteenth   and  Octave    Fifteenth ; 

thinner  but  more  pungent  tone  than  the  violin  the  Fifth,   Twelfth,  and  Larigot ;  the  Tenth 

diapason,  8  feet,  .  .  .  one  of  the  most  highly  and  Tierce ;  and  the  Mixture  Stops,  when  of 

esteemed  and  most  frequently  disposed  stops  full  or  proportional  scale,  belong  to  the  Dia- 

ifi  Continental  organs ;  the  German  gamba  is  pason-work.'     From  Hopkins  on  the  Organ, 

oBuaUy  composed  of  cylindrical  pipes.'      In  p.  131. — Translator,] 
England  till  very  recently  it  was  made  ezclu- 


Digitized  by 


Google 


94  MUSICAL  TONES  OF  FLUTE   OR  FLUE  PIPES.        part  i. 

upper  end,  in  the  aaUcional^  geffuhom,  and  spitzflote  stops.*  Their  upper  opening 
has  generally  haU  the  diameter  of  the  lower  section ;  for  the  same  length  the 
saUeional  pipe  has  the  narrowest,  and  the  spitzflote  the  widest  section.  These  pipes 
liave,  I  find,  the  property  of  rendering  some  higher  partial  tones,  from  the  Fifth 
to  the  Seventh,  comparatively  stronger  than  the  lower.  The  quality  of  tone  is 
consequently  poor,  but  peculiarly  bright. 

The  narrower  stopped  cylindrical  pipes  have  proper  tones  corresponding  to  the 
unevenly  numbered  partials  of  the  prime,  that  is,  the  third  partial  or  Twelfth,  the 
fifth  partial  or  higher  major  Third,  and  so  on.  For  the  wider  stopped  pipes,  as  for 
the  wide  open  pipes,  the  next  adjacent  proper  tones  of  the  mass  of  air  are  distinctly 
higher  than  the  corresponding  upper  partials  of  the  prime,  and  consequently  these 
upper  partials  are  very  slightly,  if  at  all,  reinforced.  Hence  wide  stopped  pipes, 
especially  when  gently  blown,  give  the  prime  tone  almost  alone,  and  they  were 

IT  therefore  previously  adduced  as  examples  of  simple  tones  (p.  6oc),  Narrow  stopped 
pipes,  on  the  other  hand,  let  the  Twelfth  be  very  distinctly  heard  at  tlie  same 
time  with  the  prime  time ;  and  have  hence  been  called  quintaten  (qmntam  tenentes),f 
When  these  pipes  are  strongly  blown  they  also  give  the  fifth  partial,  or  liigher 
major  Third,  very  distinctly.  Another  variety  of  quality  is  produced  by  the 
rohrfldte.t  Here  a  tube,  open  at  both  ends,  is  inserted  in  the  cover  of  a.  stopped 
pipe,  and  in  the  examples  I  examined,  its  length  was  that  of  an  open  pipe  giving 
the  fifth  partial  tone  of  the  prime  tone  of  the  stopped  pipe.  The  fif  h  partial  tone 
is  thus  proportionably  stronger  than  the  rather  weak  third  partial  on  these  pipes, 
and  the  quality  of  tone  becomes  peculiarly  bright.  Compared  with  open  pipes  the 
quality  of  tone  in  stopped  pipes,  where  the  evenly  numbered  partial  tones  are 
absent,  is  somewhat  hollow ;  the  wider  stopped  pipes  have  a  dull  quality  of  tone, 
especially  when  deep,  and  are  soft  and  powerless.  But  their  softness  ofiers  a  very 
effective  contrast  to  the  more  cutting  qualities  of  the  narrower  open  pipes  and  the 

f  noisy  con^ownd  stops^  of  which  I  have  already  spoken  (p.  576),  and  which,  as  is 
well  known,  form  a  compound  tone  by  uniting  many  pipes  corresponding  to  a  prime 
and  its  upper  partial  tones. 

Wooden  pipes  do  not  produce  such  a  cutting  windrush  as  metal  pipes.  Wooden 
sides  also  do  not  resist  the  agitation  of  the  waves  of  sound  so  well  as  metal  ones,  and 
hence  the  vibrations  of  higher  pitch  seem  to  be  destroyed  by  friction.  For  these 
reasons  wood  gives  a  softer,  but  duller,  less  penetrating  quality  of  tone  than  metal. 
It  is  characteristic  of  all  pipes  of  this  kind  that  they  speak  readily,  and  hence 
admit  of  great  rapidity  in  musical  divisions  and  figures,  but,  as  a  little  increase  of 
force  in  blowing  distinctly  alters  the  pitch,  their  loudness  of  tone  can  scarcely  be 
changed.  Hence  on  the  organ  forte  and  pianx)  have  to  be  produced  by  stops,  which 
regulate  the  introduction  of  pipes  with  various  qualities  of  tone,  sometimes  more, 
sometimes  fewer,  now  the  loud  and  cutting,  now  the  weak  and  soft.  The  means  of 
expression  on  this  instrument  are  therefore  somewhat  limited,  but,  on  the  other 

f  hand,  it  clearly  owes  part  of  its  magnificent  properties  to  its  power  of  sustaining 
tones  with  unsJtered  force,  undistiurbed  by  subjective  excitement. 

*  [ScMdonal^^  t9i&SlJ  Double  Duloiana,  16  oonioal  bodies,  8  feet.*    *  This  stop  is  fotmd  of 

feet  and  8  feet,  octave  siJicional,  4  feet.'    The  8,  4,  and  2  feet  length  in  Gennan  oigans.    In 

Duleiana  is'desoribed  as  *  belonging  to  the  Flate-  England  it  has  hitherto  been  made  oUefly  as  a 

work, . . .  the  pipes  mooh  smaller  in  scale  than  4-feet  stop ;  i.e.  of  principal  pitch*    The  pipes 

those  of  the  open  diapason  .  •  .  tone  peculiarly  of  the  Spitz-flute  are  sUghtly  oonicaJ,  beong 

soft  and  gentle '  {Hopkins,  p.  113).  Gwishom^  about  ^  narrower  at  top  than  at  the  mouth, 

Utorally  *  chamois  horn ;  *  in  Hopkins,  *  €k>at-  and  the  tone  is  therefore  rather  softer  than 

horn,  a  unison  open  metal  stop,  more  conical  that  of  the  cylindrical  stop,  but  of  very  pleas* 

than  the  Spitz-FlOte,  8  feet.'    *  A  member  of  ing  quality '  {ibid,  p.  140) TranskUor:\ 

the  Flute-work  and  met  with  of  8,  4,  or  2  feet  t  [See  supra  p.  33d,  note.— Tfonslotor.] 

length  in  Continental  organs.  The  pipes  of  this  t  [BohrflOie — '  Double  Stopped  Diapason  of 

stop  are  only  |  the  diameter  at  the  top  that  they  metal  pipes  with  chimneys,  16  feet,  Beed>flute« 

are  at  the  mouth ;  and  the  tone  is  consequently  Metal  Stopped  Diapason,  with  reeds,  tubes  or 

light,  but  very  clear  and   travelling '    (i6td.  chimneys,  8  feet.    Stoppied  Metal  Flute,  with 

p.  140).    SpiUflOte — ^* Spire  or  taper  flute,  a  reeds,  tubes  or  chimneys,  4  feet'  (Hopkins, 

unison  open  metal  stop  formed  of  pipes  with  pp.  444,  445)^ — TranslatcrJ] 


Digitized  by  VjOOQIC 


CHAP^  V.  6. 


MUSICAL  TONES  OP  REED  PIPES. 


95 


6.  Musical  Tones  of  Beed  Pipes. 

The  mode  of  producing  the  tones  on  these  instruments  resembles  that  used  for  I 
the  siren :  the  passage  for  the  air  being  alternately  closed  and  opened,  its  stream  is  | 
separated  into  a  series  of  individual  pulses.  This  is  effected  on  the  siren,  as  we 
have  already  seen,  by  means  of  a  rotating  disc  pierced  with  holes.  In  reed  infftm- 
ments,  elastic  plates  or  tongues  are  employed  which  are  set  in  vibration  and  thus 
alternately  close  and  open  the  aperture  in  which  they  are  fastened.  To  these 
belong — 

I.  The  reed  pipes  of  organs  and  the  vibrators  of  harmoniums.    Their  tongues, 
sdiewn  in  perspective  in  fig.  28,  A,  and  in  section  in  fig.  28,  B,  are  thin  oblong 

metal  plates,  z  z,  fastened 
''•*•  *••  to  a  brass   block,    a  a,  in 

which  there  is  a  hole,  b  b,  IT 
behind  the  tongue  and  of 
the  same  shape.  When  the 
tongue  is  in  its  position  of 
rest,  it  closes  the  hole  com- 
pletely, with  the  exception 
of  a  very  fine  chink  all  round 
its  margin.  When  in  motion 
it  oscillates  between  the  po- 
sitions marked  Z|  and  z^  in  fig.  28,  B.  In  the  position  z^  there  is  an  aperture  for 
the  stream  of  air  to  enter,  in  the  direction  shewn  by  the  arrow,  and  this  is  closed 
when  the  tongue  has  reached  the  other  extreme  position  z^.  The  tongue  shewn  / 
is  a  free  vibrator  or  anche  libre,  such  as  is  now  universally  employed.  These 
tongues  are  slightly  smaller  than  the  corresponding  opening,  so  that  they  can  bend 
inwards  without  touching  the  edges  of  the  hole.^  Formerly,  striking  vibrators  1[ 
or  reeds  were  employed,  which  on  each  oscillation  struck  against  their  fi-ame. 
Bat  as  these  produced  a  harsh  quality  of  tone  and  an  uncertain  pitch  they  have 
gone  out  of  use.f 


*  [The  quality  of  tone  prodaoed  by  the  free 
reed  can  be  greatly  modified  by  oomparatively 
slight  changes.  If  the  reed  is  quite  flat,  the 
end  not  turning  up,  as  it  does  in  fig.  28,  above, 
no  tone  can  be  produced.  If  the  size  of  the 
slit  round  the  edges  be  enlarged,  by  forcing  a 
thin  plate  of  steel  between  the  spring  and  tiie 
flange,  and  then  withdrawing  it,  the  quality  of 
tone  is  permanently  changed.  Another  change 
is  produced  by  curving  the  middle  part  up  and 
then  down  in  a  curve  of  contrary  flexure. 
Another  change  results  from  curving  the  ends 
of  the  reed  up  as  in  'American  organs*— a 
species  of  harmonium.  One  of  the  earliest  free 
reed  instruments  is  the  Chinese '  shdng,*  which 
Mr.  Hermann  Smith  thus  describes  from  his 
own  specimen.  See  also  App.  XX.  sect.  E.  'The 
body  of  the  instrument  is  in  the  form  and  size 
of  a  teacup  with  a  tightly  fitting  cover,  pierced 
with  a  series  of  holes,  arranged  in  a  circle,  to 
reoeive  a  set  of  small  pipe-like  canes,  17  in 
number,  and  of  various  lengths,  of  which  13 
are  capable  of  sounding  and  4  are  mute,  but 
necessary  for  structure.  The  lower  end  of  each 
pipe  is  fitted  with  a  little  free  reed  of  very 
delicate  workmanship,  about  half  an  indi  long, 
and  stamped  in  a  thin  metal  plate,  having  its 
tip  slightly  loaded  with  beeswax,  which  is  also 
Qsed  for  keeping  the  reed  in  position.  One 
peculiarity  to  be  noticed  is  that  the  reed  is 
quite  level  with  the  face  of  the  plate,  a  condi- 
tion in  which  modern  free  re^s  would  not 


speak.  But  this  singular  provision  is  made  to  , 
ensure  speaking  either  by  blowing  or  suction.  ' 
The  comers  of  the  reeds  are  rounded  off,  and 
thus  a  little  space  is  left  between  the  tip  of  the 
reed  and  the  frame  for  the  passage  of  air,  an 
arrangement  quite  adverse  to  the  speaking  of 
harmonium  reeds.  In  each  pipe  the  integrity 
of  the  column  of  air  is  broken  by  a  hole  in 
the  side,  a  short  distance  above  the  cup.  By 
this  strange  contrivance  not  a  single  pipe  will 
sound  to  the  wind  blown  into  the  cup  from 
a  flexible  tube,  until  its  side  hole  has  been 
covered  by  the  finger  of  the  player,  and  then 
the  pipe  gives  a  note  correspondmg  to  its  full  IT 
specJcing  length.  Whatever  be  the  speaking 
length  of  the  pipe  the  hole  is  placed  at  a  short 
distance  above  the  cup.  Its  position  has  no 
relation  to  nodal  distance,  and  it  effects  its 
purpose  by  breaking  up  the  air  column  and 
preventing  it  from  furnishing  a  proper  recipro- 
cating relation  to  the  pitch  of  the  reed.*  Hie 
instrument  thus  described  is  the  'sing'  of 
Barrow  {Travels  in  China,  1S04,  where  it  is 
well  figured  as  *a  pipe,  with  unequal  reeds 
or  bamboos'),  and  'le  petit  cheng'  of  Pdre 
Amiot  (Mimoires  concemani  VMstoire  .  .  . 
des  Chmoia  .  .  .,  1780,  vol.  vi.,  where  a '  cheng ' 
of  24  pipes  is  figured. — Translator,] 

f  [It  will  be  seen  by  App.  VII.  to  this 
edition,  end  of  sect.  A.,  ih&i  Prof.  Helmholtz 
has  somewhat  modified  his  opinion  on  this 
point,  in  consequence  of  the  information  I 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


96 


MUSICAL  TONES  OF  REED  PIPES. 


PART    I. 


The  mode  in  which  tongues  are  fastened  in  the  reed  stops  of  organs  is  shewn 
in  fig.  29,  A  and  B  below.  A  bears  a  resonant  cup  above;  B  is  a  longitu- 
dinal section ;  p  p  is  the  air  chamber 


into  which  the  wind  is  driven  ;  the 
tongue  1  is  fastened  in  the  groove  r, 
which  fits  into  the  block  s ;  d  is  the 
tuning  wire,  which  presses  against  the 
tongue,  and  being  pushed  down  shortens 
it  and  hence  sharpens  its  pitch,  and, 
conversely,  flattens  the  pitch  when  pulled 
up.  Slight  variations  of  pitch  are  thus 
easily  produced.* 
I  2 .  The  tongues  of  clarinets ,  oboes ,  and 

^  bassoons  are  constructed  in  a  somewhat 
similar  manner  and  are  cut  out  of  elastic 
reed  plates.  The  clarinet  has  a  single 
wide  tongue  which  is  fastened  before  the 
corresponding  opening  of  the  mouth- 
piece like  the  metal  tongues  previously 
described,  and  would  strike  the  frame  if 
its  excursions  were  long  enough.    But  its 

obtained  from  some  of  the  principal  English 
organ-boilders,  which  I  here  insert  from  p.  711 
of  the  first  edition  of  this  translation: — Mr. 
Willis  tells  me  that  he  never  uses  free  reeds, 
that  no  power  can  be  got  from  them,  and  that 
he  looks  upon  them  as  *  artificial  toys.' 
Messrs.  J.  W.  Walker  &  Sons  say  that  they 
m  have  also  never  used  free  reeds  for  the  forty  or 

''  more  years  that  they  have  been  in  business, 
and  consider  that  free  reeds  have  been  super- 
seded by  striking  reeds.  Mr.  Thomas  Hill 
informs  me  that  free  reeds  had  been  tried  by 
his  father,  by  M.  Cavaill^-Coll  of  Paris,  and 
others,  in  every  imaginable  way,  for  the  last 

'  thirty  or  forty  years,  and  were  abandoned  as 
'  utterly  worthless.'  But  he  mentions  that 
Schulze  (of  Paulenzelle,  Schwartzburg)  told 
him  that  he  never  saw  a  striking  reed  till 
he  came  over  to  England  in  1851,  and  that 
Walcker  (of  Ludwigsburg,  Wuertemberg)  had 
little  experience  of  them,  as  Mr.  Hill  learnt 
from  him  about  twenty  years  ago.  Mr.  Hill 
adds,  however,  that  both  these  builders  speedily 
abandoned  the  free  reed,  after  seeing  the 
English  practice  of  voicing  striking  reeds. 
This  is  corroborated  by  Mr.  Hermann  Smith's 
^  statement  (1875)  t^a*  Schulze,  in  1862,  built 
the  great  organ  at  Doncaster  with  94  stops, 
of  which  only  the  Trombone  and  its  Octave 
had  free  reeds  (see  Hopkins  on  the  Organ^ 
p.  530,  for  an  account  of  this  organ) ;  and 
that  two  years  ago  he  built  an  organ  of  64 
stops  and  4,052  pipes  for  Sheffield,  with  not 
one  free  reed;  also  that  Walcker  built  the 
great  organ  for  Ulm  cathedral,  with  6,500 
pipes  and  100  stops,  of  which  34  had  reeds, 
and  out  of  them  only  2  had  free  reeds ;  and 
that  more  recently  he  built  as  large  a  one  for 
Boston  Music  Hall,  without  more  free  reeds ; 
and  again  that  Cavaill6-Coll  quite  recently 
built  an  organ  for  Mr.  Hopwood  of  Kensington 
of  2,252  pipes  and  40  stops,  of  which  only  one 
—  the  Musette  —had  free  reeds.  He  also  says 
that  Lewis,  and  probably  most  of  the  London 
organ -builders  not  previously  mentioned,  have 
never  used  the  free  reed.  The  harshness  of  the 


Fio.  29. 


striking  reed  is  obviated  in  the  English  method 
of  voicing,  according  to  Mr.  H.  Smith,  by  so 
curving  and  manipulating  the  metal  tongue, 
that  instead  of  coming  with  a  discontinooos 

*  flap  '  from  the  fixed  extremity  down  on  to  the 
sUt  of  the  tube,  it  'rolls  itself'  down,  and 
hence  gradually  covers  the  aperture.  The  art 
of  curving  the  tongue  so  as  to  produce  this 
effect  is  very  difficult  to  acquire ;  it  is  entirely 
empirical,  and  depends  upon  the  keen  eye  and 
fine  touch  of  the  '  artist,'  who  notes  lines  and 
curves  imperceptible  to  the  uninitiated  observer, 
and  foresees  their  influence  on  the  production 
of  quality  of  tone.  Consequently,  when  an 
organ-builder  has  the  misfortune  to  lose  his 

*  reed-voicer,'  he  has  always  great  difficulty  in 
replacing  him. — Translator.] 

*  [It  should  be  observed  that  fig.  29,  A, 
shews  tkfree  reed,  and  fig.  29,  B,  a  striking  reed  ; 
and  that  the  tuning  wire  is  right  in  fig.  29,  B, 
because  it  presses  the  reed  against  the  edges  of 
its  groove  and  hence  shortens  it,  but  it  is  wrong 
in  fig.  29,  A,  for  the  reed  being  free  would  strike 
against  the  wire  and  rattle.  For  free  reeds  a 
clip  is  used  which  grasps  the  reed  on  both  sides 
and  thus  limits  its  vibrating  length. 

Fig.  28,  p.  956,  shews  the  vibrator  of  aA 
harmonium,  not  of  an  organ  pipe.  The  figures 
are  the  same  as  in  all  the  German  editions. — 
Translator.] 

Digitized  by  V^ O OQ^  IC 


OHAP.  V.  6. 


TONES  OF  REED  PIPES. 


97 


Pio. 


excursions  are  small,  and  the  pressure  of  the  lips  brings  it  just  near  enough  to 
make  the  chink  sufficiently  small  without  allowing  it  to  strike.  For  the  oboe  and 
bassoon  two  reeds  or  tongues  of  the  same  kind  are  placed  opposite  each  other  at  the 
end  of  the  mouthpiece.  They  are  separated  by  a  narrow  chink,  and  by  blowing  are 
pressed  near  enough  to  close  the  chink  whenever  they  swing  inwards. 

3.  Membranous  tongues, — The  peculiarities  of  these  tongues  are  best  studied 
on  those  artificially  constructed.    Gut  the  end  of  a  wooden  or  gutta-percha  tube 

obliquely  on  both  sides,  as  shewn  in  fig.  30, 
leaving  two  nearly  rectangular  points  standing 
between  the  two  edges  which  are  cut  obUquely. 
Then  gently  stretch  strips  of  vulcanised  india- 
rubber  over  the  two  obUque  edges,  so  as  to  leave 
a  small  sUt  between  them,  and  fasten  them  with 
a  thread.    A  reed  mouthpiece  is  thus  constructed  ^ 
which  may  be  connected  in  any  way  with  tubes 
or  other  air  chambers.    When  the  membranes 
bend  inwards  the  slit  is  closed ;  when  outwards, 
it  is  open.    Membranes  which  are  fastened  in     | 
this  obliqiie  manner  speak  much  better  than  those  which  are  laid  at  right  angles     > 
to  the  axis  of  the  tube,  as  Johannes  Miiller  proposed,  for  in  the  latter  case  they 
require  to  be  bent  outwards  by  the  air  before  they  can  begin  to  open  and  shut 
alternately.    Membranous  tongues  of  the  kind  proposed  may  be  blown  either  in 
the  direction  of  the  arrows  or  in  the  opposite  direction.    In  the  first  case  they  open 
the  slit  when  they  move  towards  the  air  chamber,  that  is,  towards  the  further  end 
of  the  conducting  tube.    Tongues  of  this  kind  I  distinguish  as  striking  inwards,  i^ 
When  blown  they  always  give  deeper  tones  than  they  would  do  if  allowed  to 
vibrate  freely,  that  is,  without  being  connected  with  an  air  chamber.    The  tongues 
of  organ  pipes,  harmoniums,  and  wooden  wind  instruments  already  mentioned,  ^ 
are  likewise  always  arranged  to  strike  inwards.    But  both  membranous  and  metal 
tongues  may  be  arranged  so  as  to  act  against  the  stream  of  aur,  and  hence  to  open 
when  they  move  towards  the  outer  opening  of  the  instrument.    I  then  say  that  they 
strike  (mtwards.    The  tones  of  tongues  which  strike  outwards  are  always  sharper   ^ 
than  those  of  isolated  tongues. 

Only  two  kinds  of  membranous  tongues  have  to  be  considered  as  musical  in- 
struments :  the  human  lips  in  brass  instruments,  and  the  human  larynx  in  singing. 
The  lips  must  be  considered  as  very  shghtly  elastic  membranous  tongues, 
loaded  with  much  inelastic  tissue  containing  water,  and  they  would  consequently 
vibrate  very  slowly,  if  they  could  be  brought  to  vibrate  by  themselves.  In  brass 
instruments  they  form  membranous  tongues  which  strike  outwards,  and  conse- 
quently by  the  above  rule  produce  tones  sharper  than  their  proper  tones.  But  as 
they  offer  very  slight  resistance,  they  are  readily  set  in  motion,  by  the  alternate 
pressure  of  the  vibrating  column  of  air,  when  used  with  brass  instruments.*  m 


*  [Mr.  D.  J.  Blaikley  (manager  of  Messrs. 
fioosey  A  Ck).*B  Military  Musioal  Instrament 
Manoiactory,  who  has  studied  all  such  instm- 
ments  theoretically  as  well  as  practically,  and 
read  many  papers  npon  them,  to  some  of  which 
I  shall  have  to  refer)  finds  that  this  statement 
does  not  represent  his  own  sensations  when 
playing  the  horn.  *  The  lips,'  he  says,  *  do  not 
▼ibrate  throoghoat  their  whole  length,  bat  only 
through  a  certain  length  determined  by  the 
diameter  of  the  cap  of  the  mouthpiece.  Pro- 
bably also  the  vibrating  length  can  be  modified 
by  the  mere  pinch,  at  least  this  is  the  sensa- 
tion I  experience  when  sounding  high  notes  on  a 
largemouthpiece.  The  compass  (about  4  octaves) 
possible  on  a  given  mouthpiece  is  much  greater 
than  that  of  any  one  register  of  the  voice,  and 


the  whole  range  of  brass  instruments  played 
thus  with  the  hps  is  about  one  octave  greater 
than  the  whole  range  of  the  human  voioe  from 
basso  profundo  to  the  highest  soprano.  That 
the  lips,  acting  as  the  vocal  chords  do,  can 
themselves  vibrate  rapidly  when  supported  by 
the  rim  of  a  mouthpiece,  may  be  proved,  for  if 
such  a  rim,  unconnected  witii  any  resonating 
tube,  be  held  against  the  lips,  various  notes  of 
the  scale  can  be  produced  very  faintly,  the  dif- 
ficulty being  to  maintain  steadiness  of  pitch 
{Philos.  Mag,,  Aug.  1878,  p.  2).  The  qffke  of 
the  air  in  the  tube  in  relation  to  the  lips  (leav- 
ing out  of  consideration  its  work  as  a  resonant 
body,  intensifying  and  modifying  the  tone)  is 
to  act  as  a  pendulum  governor  in  facilitating 
the  maintenance  (not  the  origination)  of  a 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


98  TONES  OF  EEED  PIPES.  pakt  i. 

In  tlie  larynx,  the  elastic  vocal  chords  act  as  membranous  tongues.  They  are 
^  stretched  across  the  windpipe,  from  front  to  back,  like  the  india-rubber  strips  in 
fig,  30  (p.  97a),  and  leave  a  small  slit,  the  glottis,  between  them.  They  have  the 
advantage  over  all  artificially  constructed  tongues  of  allowing  the  width  of  their  slit, 
their  tension,  and  even  their  form  to  be  altered  at  pleasure  with  extraordinary 
rapidity  and  certainty,  at  the  same  time  that  the  resonant  tube  formed  by  the 
opening  of  the  mouth  admits  of  much  variety  of  form,  so  that  many  more  qualities 
of  tone  can  be  thus  produced  than  on  any  instrument  of  artificial  construction.  If 
the  vocal  chords  are  examined  from  above  with  a  laryngoscope,  while  producing  a 
tone,  they  will  be  seen  to  make  very  large  vibrations  for  the  deeper  breast  voice, 
shutting  the  glottis  tightly  whenever  they  strike  inwards. 

The  pitch  of  the  various  reeds  or  tongues  just  mentioned  is  altered  in  very 
different  manners.  The  metal  tongues  of  the  organ  and  harmonium  are  always 
H  intended  to  produce  one  single  tone  apiece.  On  the  motion  of  these  comparatively 
heavy  and  stiff  tongues,  the  pressure  of  the  vibrating  air  has  very  small  influence, 
and  their  pitch  within  the  instrument  is  consequently  not  much  different  from  that 
of  the  isolated  tongues.  There  must  be  at  least  one  tongue  for  each  note  on  such 
instruments. 

In  wooden  wind  instruments,  a  single  tongue  has  to  serve  for  the  whole  series 
of  notes.  But  the  tongues  of  these  instruments  are  made  of  light  elastic  wood, 
which  is  easily  set  in  motion  by  the  alternating  pressure  of  the  vibrating  column 
of  air,  and  swings  sympathetically  with  it.  Such  instruments,  therefore,  in 
addition  to  those  very  high  tones,  which  nearly  correspond  to  the  proper  tones  of 
their  tongues,  can,  as  theory  and  experience  alike  shew,  also  produce  deep  tones  of 
a  very  different  pitch,*  because  the  waves  of  air  which  arise  in  the  tube  of  the  in- 
strument excite  an  alternation  in  the  pressure  of  air  adjacent  to  the  tongue  itself 
sufficiently  powerful  to  make  it  vibrate  sensibly.  Now  in  a  vibrating  column  of 
II  air  the  alteration  of  pressure  is  greatest  where  the  velocity  of  the  particles  of  air  is 
smallest ;  and  since  the  velocity  is  always  null,  that  is  a  minimum,  at  the  end  of  a 
closed  tube,  such  as  a  stopped  organ  pipe,  and  the  alteration  of  pressure  in  that 
place  is  consequently  a  maximum,  the  tones  of  these  reed  pipes  must  be  the  same  as 
those  which  the  resonant  tube  alone  would  produce,  if  it  were  stopped  at  the  place 
where  the  tongue  is  placed,  and  were  blown  as  a  stopped  pipe.  In  musical  practice, 
then,  such  tones  of  the  instrument  as  correspond  to  the  proper  tones  of  the  tongue 
are  not  used  at  all,  because  they  are  very  high  and  screaming,  and  their  pitch  can- 
not be  preserved  with  sufficient  steadiness  when  the  tongue  is  wet.  The  only 
tones  produced  are  considerably  deeper  than  the  proper  tone  of  the  tongue,  and 
have  their  pitches  determined  by  the  length  of  the  column  of  air,  which  corresponds 
to  the  proper  tones  of  the  stopped  pipe. 

The  clarinet  has  a  cylindrical  tube,  the  proper  tones  of  which  correspond  to 

the  third,  fifth,  seventh,  &c.,  partial  tone  of  the  prime.     By  altering  the  style  of 

%  blowing,  it  is  possible  to  pass  from  the  prime  to  the  Twelfth  or  the  higher  major 

Third.    The  acoustic  length  of  the  tube  may  also  be  altered  by  opening  the  side 

periodic  vibration  of  the  lips.    Prof.  Helmholtz  which  he  produced  a  tone  of  40  vib.,  the  tone 

does  not  say  above  what  produces  the  alternate  was,  even  at  that  depth,  remarkably  rich  and 

pressure,  and  I  can  conceive  no  source  for  it  but  fine,  owing  to  the  large  and  deep  cup  extinguiah- 

a  periodic  vibration  of  the  lips  of  a  time  suited  ing  the  beating  upper  partials.    Mr.  Blaikley 

to  the  particular  note  required.'    The  depth  of  also  drew  my  attention  to  the  fact  that  where 

the  cup  is  also  important:—*  The  shallower  and  the  tube  opens  out  into  the  cup,  there  must 

more  *'  cup-like  "  the  cup,'  says  Mr.  Blaikley,  be  no  sharp  shoulder,  but  that  the  edge  must 

*  the  greater  the  strength  of  the  upper  partials.  be  carefully  rounded  ofif,  otherwise  there  is  a 

Compare  the  deep   and  narrow  cup  of  the  great  loss  of  power  to  the  blower.    In  the  ease 

French  horn  with  weak  upper  partials,  and  of  the  French  horn  the  cup  is  very  long  and 

the  wide  and  shallow  cup  of  the  trumpet  with  almost  tapers  into  the  tube.— SVon^^tor.} 
strongupperpurtials.'— (MS.  communications.)  *  See  Helmholtz,  Verhandlungen  des  na- 

Mr.  Blaikley  kindly  sounded  for  me  the  same  turhisiorischen  medidnischen  Vereins  bii  Hei- 

instrument  with  different  mouthpieces  or  cups,  delberg^  July   26,  1861,  in  the  Heidelberger 

to  shew  the  great  difference  of  quality  they  Jahrbiicher.    Poggendorff's    Annalen^     1861. 

produce.    In  the  great  bass    bombardon  on  [Reproduced  in  part  in  App.  VII.  sect.  B.,  I.j 


Digitized  by  V^OOQIC 


CHAP.  V.   6. 


TONES  OF  REED  PIPES. 


99 


holes  of  the  clarinet,  in  which  case  the  vibrating  column  of  air  is  principally  that 
between  the  mouthpiece  and  the  uppermost  open  side  hole.* 

The  oboe  (hautbois)  and  bassoon  (fagotto)  have  conical  tubes  which  are  closed  up 
to  the  vertex  of  their  cone,  and  have  proper  tones  that  are  the  same  as  those  of 
open  tubes  of  the  same  length.  Hence  the  tones  of  both  of  these  instruments 
nearly  correspond  to  those  of  open  pipes.  By  overblowing  they  give  the  Octave, 
Twelfth,  second  Octave,  and  so  on,  of  the  prime  tone.  Intermediate  tones  are 
produced  by  opening  side  holes. 

The  older  horns  and  trumpets  consist  of  long  conical  bent  tubes,  without  keys 
or  side  holes.f  They  can  produce  such  tones  only  as  correspond  to  the  proper 
tones  of  the  tube,  and  these  again  are  the  natural  harmonic  upper  partials  of  the 
prime.  But  as  the  prime  tone  of  such  a  long  tube  is  very  deep,  the  upper  partial 
tones  in  the  middle  parts  of  the  scale  lie  rather  close  together,  especially  in  the 
extremely  long  tubes  of  the  hom,t  so  that  they  give  most  of  the  degrees  of  the  scale.  ^ 


*  [Mr.  D.  J.  Blaikley  obligingly  furnished  me 
with  the  Bobstanoe  of  the  following  remarks  on 
clarinets,  and  repeated  his  experiments  before 
me  in  May  1884.  The  ordinary  form  of  the 
clarinet  is  not  wholly  cylindrical.  It  is  slightly 
eonstricted  at  the  mouthpiece  and  provided 
with  a  spreading  bell  at  the  other  end.  The 
modification  of  form  by  key  and  finger  holes 
also  must  not  be  neglected.  On  a  cylindrical 
pipe  played  with  the  lips,  the  evenly  numbered 
partials  are  quite  inaudible.  When  a  clarinet 
mouthpiece  was  added  I  found  traces  of  the 
4th  and  6th  partials  beating  with  my  forks. 
But  on  the  clarinet  with  the  bell,  the  2nd, 
4th,  and  6th  partials  were  distinct,  and  I  could 
obtain  beats  from  them  with  my  forks.  Mr. 
Blaikley  brought  them  out  (i)  by  bead  and 
diaphragm  resonators  tuned  to  them  (fig.  15, 
p.  42a),  which  I  also  witnessed,  (2)  by  an  irre- 
gularly-shaped tubular  resonator  sunk  gra- 
dually in  water,  on  which  I  also  heard  them. 


(3)  by  beats  with  an  harmonium  with  a  con- 
stant blast,  which  I  also  heard.  On  the  cylin- 
drical tube  all  the  unevenly  numbered  partials 
are  in  tune  when  played  as  primes  of  inde- 
pendent harmonic  notes.  On  the  clarinet 
only  the  3rd  partial,  or  2nd  proper  tone,  can 
be  used  as  the  prime  of  an  independent  har- 
monic tone.  The  3rd,  4th,  and  5th  proper 
tones  of  the  instrument,  are  sufficiently  near 
in  pitch  to  the  5th,  7th,  and  9th  partials  of 
the  fundamental  tone  for  these  latter  to  be 
greatly  strengthened  by  resonance,  but  the 
agreement  is  not  close  enough  to  allow  of  the 
higher  proper  tones  being  used  as  the  primes 
of  independent  harmonic  compound  tones, 
fience  practically  only  the  3rd  harmonics, 
or  Twelfths,  are  used  on  the  clarinet.  The 
following  table  of  the  relative  intensity  of  the  m 
partials  of  a  Bb  clarinet  was  given  by  Mr. 
Blaikley  in  the  Proc.  of  ttu  Uus.  Assn.  for 
1877-8,  p.  84:-- 


PAKTIilLg— Bb   OLAHIHETa 

. 

Pitch 

I 

a 

3 

4 

5 

6 

7 

8,  Ac. 

f 

/ 

^ 

/ 

V 

mf 

P 

... 

6b 

/ 

/ 

P 

mf 

mf 

PP 

a 

/ 

/ 

P 

mf 

... 

mf 

PP 

9 

/ 

8 

/ 

... 

mf 

mf 

P 

PP 

f 

/ 

•0 

/ 

... 

fnf 

P 

mf 

PP 

«b 

/ 

t 

mf 

... 

P 

P 

mf 

PP 

d 

/ 

^ 

mi 

P 

mf 

P 

P 

PP 

Where/  means  forte,  mf  mezzoforte, 

t  [Such  brass  tubes  are  first  worked  unbent 
from  cylindrical  brass  tubes,  by  putting  solid 
steel  cores  of  the  required  form  inside,  and  then 
drawing  them  through  a  hole  in  a  piece  of 
lead,  which  yields  enough  for  the  tube  to  pass 
through,  bat  presses  the  brass  firmly  enough 
against  the  core  to  make  the  tube  assume  the 
proper  form.  Afterwards  the  tube  is  filled 
with  lead,  and  then  bent  into  the  required  coils, 
after  which  the  lead  is  melted  out.  The  in- 
ttroments  are  also  not  conical  in  the  strict 
Bense  of  the  word,  but  *  approximate  in  form 
to  the  hyperboHe  cone,  where  the  axis  of  the 
instrument  is  an  asymptote,  and  the  vertex  is 
at  a  great  or  even  an  infinite  distance  from 
the  bdl  end.'  From  information  furnished  by 
Mr.  BUdkley.— 5rV»fwtotor.] 

X  The  tube  of  the  Waldhom  [foresthom, 
Notes       .        .        .    e'b       /       ^ 
Just  cents       .       .    o,      204,    386, 
Harmonic  cents       .     o,      204,    386, 
Hannonics,  No.       .     8,       9,       10, 


p  piano,  |7p  pianissimo.— TVanaZator.] 

hunting  horn  of  the  Germans,  answering  to 
our  French  horn]  is,  according  to  Zamminer 
[p.  3 1 2],  13*4  feet  long.  Its  proper  prime  tone  ^ 
is  E,b.  This  and  the  next  Eb  are  not  used, 
but  only  the  other  tones,  Bb,  eb,  gr,  6b,  i'b-, 
«'b,  /,  gr',  a'b  +  ,  6'b,  Ac.  [Mr.  Blaikley 
kindly  sounded  for  me  the  harmonics  8,  9,  10, 
II,  12,  13,  14  on  an  E^b  French  horn.  The 
result  was  almost  precisely  320,  360,  400,  440, 
480,  520,  560  vib.,  that  is  the  exact  harmonics 
for  the  prime  tone  40  vib.  to  which  it  was 
tuned,  the  pitch  of  English  military  musical 
instruments  being  as  nearly  as  possible  cf  269, 
efb  3i9'9f  a'  452*4.  This  scale  was  not  com- 
pleted because  the  15th  and  i6th  harmonics 
600  and  640  vib.  would  have  been  too  high  for 
me  to  measure.  Expressed  in  cents  we  may 
compare  this  scale  with  just  intonation  thus  :-- 

a'b  6'b  c"  d"b  d"  «"b 
498,  702,  884,  996,  1088,  1200 
551,      702,     841.     9&9.     1088,    1200  T 

II,  12,         13,         14,    Digii5^dbyV6lOOyiL 

H  2   ^ 


loo  TONES  OF  REED  PIPES.  pabt  i. 

The  trumpet  is  restricted  to  these  natural  tones.  But  by  introducing  the  hand 
into  the  bell  of  the  French  horn  and  thus  partly  closing  it,  and  by  lengthening 
the  tube  of  the  trombone,*  it  was  possible  in  some  degree  to  supply  the  missing 
tones  and  improve  the  faulty  ones.  In  later  times  trumpets  and  horns  have  been 
frequently  supplied  with  keys  f  to  supply  the  missing  tones,  but  at  some  expense 
of  power  in  the  tone  and  the  brilliancy  in  its  quaUty.  The  vibrations  of  the  air 
in  these  instruments  are  unusually  powerful,  and  require  the  resistance  of  firm, 
smooth,  unbroken  tubes  to  preserve  their  strength.  In  the  use  of  brass  inatra- 
ments,  the  different  form  and  tension  of  the  lips  of  the  player  act  only  to  determine 
which  of  the  proper  tones  of  the  tube  shall  speak ;  the  pitch  of  the  individual 
tones  is  almost  t  entirely  independent  of  the  tension  of  the  lips. 

On  the  other  hand,  in  the  larynx  the  tension  of  the  vocal  chords,  which  here 
form  the  membranous  tongues,  is  itself  variable,  and  determines  the  pitch  of 

^  the  tone.  The  air  chambers  connected  with  the  larynx  are  not  adapted  for 
materially  altering  the  tone  of  the  vocal  chords.  Their  walls  are  so  yielding  that 
they  cannot  allow  the  formation  of  vibrations  of  the  air  within  them  sufficiently 
powerful  to  force  the  vocal  chords  to  oscillate  with  a  period  which  is  different  firom 
that  required  by  their  own  elasticity.  The  cavity  of  the  mouth  is  also  far  too 
short,  and  generally  too  widely  open  for  its  mass  of  air  to  have  material  influence 
on  the  pitch. 

In  addition  to  the  tension  of  the  vocal  chords  (which  can  be  increased  not 
only  by  separating  the  points  of  their  insertion  in  the  cartilages  of  the  larynx,  but 
also  by  voluntarily  stretching  the  muscular  fibres  within  them),  their  thickness 
seems  also  to  be  variable.  Much  soft  watery  inelastic  tissue  lies  underneath  the 
elastic  fibrils  proper  and  the  muscular  fibres  of  the  vocal  chords,  and  in  the  breast 
voice  this  probably  acts  to  weight  them  and  retard  their  vibrations.  The  head 
voice  is  probably  produced  by  drawing  aside  the  mucous  coat  below  the  chords, 

^  thus  rendering  the  edge  of  the  chords  sharper,  and  the  weight  of  the  vibrating 
part  less,  while  the  elasticity  is  unaltered.§ 

Hence  the  Fourth  a'b  was  53  cents  (33  :  32)  trombone  can  be  altered  at  will,  and  chosen 

too  sharp,  and  the  Sixth  c"  was  43  cents  to  make  its  harmonics  produce  a  jnst  scale. 

(40  :  39)  too  fiat,  and  they  were  conseqaently  Some  trumpets  also  are  made  with  a  sdiort 

unusable  without  modification  by  the  hand.  slide  worked  by  two  fingers  one  way,   and 

The  minor  Seventh  <2"b  was  too  fiat  by  27  cents  returning  to  its  position  by  a  spring.    Snch 

(64  :  63),    but    unless    played   in    (intended)  instruments  are  sometimes  used  by  first-rate 

unison  against  the  just  form,  it  produces  a  players,  such  as  Harper,  the  late  celebrated 

better  effect.    *  In  trumpets,  strictly  so  called,'  trumpeter,  and  his  son.    But,  as  Mr.  Blaiklej 

says  Mr.  Blaikley,  *  a  great  portion  of  the  length  informed  me,  an  extremely  small  percentage 

is  cylindrical  and  the  bell  curves  out  hyper-  of  the  trumpets  sold  have  slides.    At  ptesent 

boliciJly,  the    two   lowest    partials  are    not  the  piston  brass  instruments  have  nearly  driven 

required  as  a  rule  and  are  not  strictly  in  all  slides,  except  the  trombone,  out  of  the  field, 

tune,  so  the  series  of  partials  may  be  taken  — Translator.] 

as  about  75,  1-90,  3,  4i  5i  6,  7,  8,  Ac,  all  the  f  P"*^©  ^^7^  *ro  nearly  obsolete,  and  have 

upper  notes  being  brought  into  tune  by  modi-  been  replaced  by  pistons  which  open  vahres, 

m  ficationsintheformof  thebellinagoodinstru-  and  thus  temporarily  increase  the  length  of 

^  ment.'    The  length  of  the  French  horn  varies  the  tube,  so  as  to  make  the  note  blown  i,  2, 

with  the  *  crook  *  which  determines  its  pitch,  or  3  Semitones  fiatter.    These  can  also  be 

The  following  contains  the  length  in  English  used  in  combination,  but  are  then  not  so  trae. 

inches  for  each  crook,  as  given  by  Mr.  Blaikley:  This   is   tantamount    to   an  imperfect   slide 

Bb   (alto)  108,  Afi    114J,  Ab   I2ij^,  O  I28|,  action.    Instruments  of   this   kind  are  now 

F  144^,  ^H  153*  Eb  102,  DQ  171^,  C  192},  much  used   in  all  military  bands,  and  are 

Bb  (basso)  2i6|,  hence  the  length  varies  from  made  of  very  different  sizes  and  pitohea. — 

9  ft.  to  18  ft.  I  inch.    By  a  curious  error  in  Translator.] 

all  the  German  editions,  Zamminer  is  said  to  X  (^^^  ^y  no  means  *  quite.*    It  is  possible 

make  the  length  of  the  .^b  Waldhom  27  feet,  to  blow  out  of  tune,  and  to  a  small  extent 

or  the  length  of  the  wave  of  the  lowest  note,  temper  the  harmonics. — Translator.] 

in  place  of  his  13*4  feet.    Zamminer,  however,  §  [On  the  subject  of  the  registers  of  the 

says  that  the  instrument  is  named  from  the  human  voice  and  its  production  generally,  see 

Octave  above  the  lowest  note,  and  that  hence  Lennox  Browne  and  Emil  Behnke,  Voice,  Song^ 

the  wave-length  of  this  Octave  is  the  length  of  and  Speech  (Sampson  Low,  London,   1883, 

the  horn. — Translator.]  pp.  322).      This  work  contains  not   merely 

*  [A  large  portion  of  the  trombone  is  com-  accurate  drawings  of  the  larynx  in  the  different 

posed  of  a  double  narrow  cylindrical  tube  on  registers,  but  4    laryngoscopic    photographs 

which  another  slides,  so  that  the  length  of  the  from  Mr.  Behnke's  own  larynx.     A  register 


Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


OHAP.  y.  6. 


TONES  OP  REED  PIPES. 


lOI 


We  now  proceed  to  investigate  the  quality  of  tone  produced  on  reed  pipes,  ! 
which  is  onr  proper  subject.  The  sound  in  these  pipes  is  excited  by  intermittent 
pulses  of  air,  which  at  each  swing  break  through  the  opening  that  is  closed  by 
ihe  tongue  of  the  reed.  A  freely  vibrating  tongue  has  far  too  small  a  surface  to 
communicate  any  appreciable  quantity  of  sonorous  motion  to  the  surrounding  air ;  / 
and  it  is  as  little  able  to  excite  the  air  inclosed  in  pipes.  The  sound  seems  to  be 
reaUy  produced  by  pulses  of  air,  as  in  the  siren,  where  the  metal  plate  that  opens 
and  closes  the  orifice  does  not  vibrate  at  all.  By  the  alternate  opening  and  closing 
of  a  passage,  a  continuous  influx  of  air  is  changed  into  a  periodic  motion,  capable 
of  affecting  the  air.  Like  any  other  periodic  motion  of  the  air,  the  one  thus 
produced  can  also  be  resolved  into  a  series  of  simple  vibrations.  We  have  already 
remarked  that  the  number  of  terms  in  such  a  series  will  increase  with  the  discon- 
tinuity of  the  motion  to  be  thus  resolved  (p.  Z4^)'  ^^^  ^^  motion  of  the  air  which 
passes  through  a  siren,  or  past  a  vibrating  tongue,  is  discontinuous  in  a  very  high  ^ 
degree,  since  the  individual  pulses  of  air  must  be  generally  separated  by  complete 
pauses  during  the  closures  of  the  opening.  Free  tongues  without  a  resonance 
tube,  in  which  all  the  individual  simple  tones  of  the  vibration  which  they  excite 
in  the  air  are  given  off  freely  to  the  surrounding  atmosphere,  have  consequently 
always  a  very  sharp,  cutting,  jarring  quahty  of  tone,  and  we  can  really  hear  with 
either  armed  or  unarmed  ears  a  long  series  of  strong  and  clear  partial  tones  up 
to  the  i6th  or  20th,  and  there  are  evidently  still  higher  partials  present,  although 
it  is  difficult  or  impossible  to  distinguish  them  from  each  other,  because  tliey  do 
not  lie  so  much  as  a  Semitone  apart.*  This  whirring  of  dissonant  partial  tones 
makes  the  musical  quality  of  free  tongues  very  disagreeable.f  A  tone  thus  pro- 
duced also  shews  that  it  is  really  due  to  puffs  of  air.  I  have  examined  the  vibra- 
ting tongue  of  a  reed  pipe,  like  that  in  fig.  28  (p.  956),  when  in  action  with  the 
vibration  microscope  of  Lissajous,  in  order  to  determine  the  vibrational  foim  of 
the  tongue,  and  I  found  that  the  tongue  performed  perfectly  regular  simple  vibra-  f 
tions.  Hence  it  would  communicate  to  the  air  merely  a  simple  tone  and  not  a 
compound  tone,  if  the  sound  were  directly  produced  by  its  own  vibrations. 

The  intensity  of  the  upper  partial  tones  of  a  free  tongue,  unconnected  with  a 
resonance  tube,  and  their  relation  to  the  prime^  are  greatly  dependent  on  the 


18  defined  as  'a  series  of  tones  produced  by 
the  same  mechanism  *  (p.  163).  The  names  of 
the  registers  adopted  are  those  introduced 
by  the  late  John  Gurwen  of  the  Tonio  Sol-fa 
movement.  They  depend  on  the  appearance  of 
the  glottis  and  vocal  chords,  and  are  as  follows : 
I.  Lower  thick,  2.  Upper  thick  (both  *  chest 
voice '),  3.  Lower  thin  ('  high  chest  *  voice  in 
men),  4.  Upper  thin  (* falsetto*  in  women), 
5.  Small  (*  head  voice '  in  women).  The  extent 
of  the  registers  are  stated  to  be  (p.  171) 

1.  lower  thick.    3.  upper  thick.    Z.  lower  thin. 
/Mew      ^toa,         6  to/,  /toe" 

lWoMiN«   toe',        d'to/,  /toe" 

L  lower  thick.    3.  upper  thick.    8.  lower  thin. 


WoaCKK  0»LY, 


<i"to/'. 

4.  upper  thin. 


/'to/" 

A.  small. 


The  mechanism  is  as  follows  (pp.  163- 171) : — 

1 .  Upper  thick.  The  hindmost  points  of  the 
pyramids  (arytenoid  cartilages)  close  together, 
an  elliptical  slit  between  the  vocal  ligaments 
(or  chords),  which  vibrate  through  their  whole 
length,  breadth,  and  thickness  fully,  loosely, 
and  visibly.    The  lid  (epiglottisj  is  low. 

2.  Upper  thick.  The  elliptical  chink  dis- 
appears and  becomes  linear.  The  lid  (epiglottis) 
rises ;  the  vocal  ligaments  are  stretched. 

3.  Lower  thin.  The  lid  (epiglottis)  is  more 
raised,  so  as  to  shew  the  cushion  below  it,  the 
whole  larynx  and  the  insertions  of  the  vocal 


ligaments  in  the  shield  (thyroid)  cartilage. 
The  vocal  ligaments  are  quite  still,  and  their 
vibrations  are  confined  to  the  thin  inner  edges. 
The  vocal  ligaments  are  made  thiwner  and 
transparent,  as  shown  by  illumination  from 
below.    Male  voices  oease  here. 

4.  Upper  thin.  An  elliptical  slit  again  forms 
between  the  vooal  ligaments.  When  this  is 
used  by  men  it  gives  the  falsetto  arising  from 
the  upper  thin  being  carried  below  its  true 
place.  This  slit  is  gradually  reduced  in  size 
as  the  contralto  and  soprano  voices  ascend.       mr 

5.  Small.  The  back  part  of  the  glottis  ' 
contracts  for  at  least  two-thirds  of  its  length, 
the  vocal  ligaments  being  pressed  together  so 
tightly  that  scarcely  any  trace  of  a  slit  remains, 
and  no  vibrations  are  visible.  The  front  part 
opens  as  an  oval  chink,  and  the  edges  of  this 
vibrate  so  markedly  that  the  outline  is  blurred. 
The  drawings  of  the  two  last  registers  (pp.  168- 
169)  were  made  from  laryngoscopic  examina- 
tion of  a  lady. 

Beference  should  be  made  to  the  book 
itself  for  full  explanations,  and  the  reader 
should  especially  consult  Mr.  Behnke*s  admir- 
able little  work  The  Mechanism  of  the  Human 
Voice  (Curwen,  3rd  ed.,  1881,  pp.  125). — Trans- 
lator,] 

*  [See  footnote  f  p.  S6d^.— Translator,] 
f  [The  cheap  little  mouth  harmonicons  ex- 
hibit this  dleci  very  well.— IVaYuIa^or.] 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


I02  TONES  OF  REED  PIPES.  pabt  i. 

nature  of  the  tongue,  its  position  with  respeot  to  its  frame,  the  tightness  with 
which  it  closes,  &c.  Striking  tongues  which  produce  the  most  discontinuous  pulses 
of  air,  also  produce  the  most  cutting  quality  of  tone.*  The  shorter  the  puff  of  air, 
and  hence  the  more  sudden  its  action,  the  greater  number  of  high  upper  partials 
should  we  expect,  exactly  as  we  find  in  the  siren,  according  to  Seebeck's  investi- 
gations. Hard,  unyielding  material,  like  that  of  brass  tongues,  will  produce 
pulses  of  air  which  are  much  more  disconnected  than  those  formed  by  soft  and 
yielding  substances.  This  is  probably  the  reason  why  the  smging  tones  of  the 
human  voice  are  softer  than  all  others  which  are  produced  by  reed  pipes.  Never- 
theless the  number  of  upper  partial  tones  in  the  human  voice,  when  used  in 
emphatic  forte,  is  very  great,  and  they  reach  distinctly  and  powerfully  up  to  the 
four- times  accented  [or  quarter-foot]  Octave  (p.  26a).  To  this  we  shall  have  to 
return. 

%  The  tones  of  tongues  are  essentially  changed  by  the  addition  of  resonance 
tubes,  because  they  reinforce  and  hence  give  prominence  to  those  upper  partial 
tones  which  correspond  to  the  proper  tones  of  these  tubes.f  In  this  case  the 
resonance  tubes  must  be  considered  as  closed  at  the  point  where  the  tongue  is 
inserted.^: 

A  brass  tongue  such  as  is  used  in  organs,  and  tuned  to  h^,  was  appUed  to  one 
of  my  larger  spherical  resonators,  also  tuned  to  ^,  instead  of  to  its  usual  resonance 
tube.  After  considerably  increasing  the  pressure  of  wind  in  the  bellows,  the 
tongue  spoke  somewhat  flatter  than  usual,  but  with  an  extraordinarily  fuU,  beautiful, 
soft  tone,  £rom  which  almost  all  upper  partials  were  absent.  Very  httle  wind  was 
used,  but  it  was  under  high  pressure.  In  this  case  the  prime  tone  of  the  compound 
was  in  unison  with  the  resonator,  which  gave  a  powerful  resonance,  and  conse- 
quently the  prime  tone  had  also  great  power.  None  of  the  higher  partial  tones 
could  be  reinforced.    The  theory  of  the  vibrations  of  air  in  the  sphere  further 

IF  shews  that  the  greatest  pressure  must  occur  in  the  sphere  at  the  moment  that  the 
tongue  opens.  Hence  arose  the  necessity  of  strong  pressure  in  the  bellows  to  over- 
come the  increased  pressure  in  the  sphere,  and  yet  not  much  wind  really  passed. 

If  instead  of  a  glass  sphere,  resonant  tubes  are  employed,  which  admit  of  a 
greater  number  of  proper  tones,  the  resulting  musical  tones  are  more  complex. 
In  the  clarinet  we  have  a  cylindrical  tube  which  by  its  resonance  reinforces  tlie 
uneven  partial  tones.§  The  conical  tubes  of  the  oboe,  bassoon,  trumpet,  and 
French  horn,  on  the  other  hand,  reinforce  all  the  harmonic  upper  partial  tones  of 
the  compound  up  to  a  certain  height,  determined  by  the  incapacity  of  the  tubes 
to  resound  for  waves  of  sound  that  are  not  much  longer  than  the  width  of  the 
opening.  By  actual  trial  I  found  only  unevenly  numbered  partial  tones,  distinct  to 
the  seventh  inclusive,  in  the  notes  of  the  clarinet,§  whereas  on  other  instruments, 
which  have  conical  tubes,  I  found  the  evenly  numbered  partials  also.  I  have  not  yet 
had  an  opportunity  of  making  observations  on  the  further  differences  of  quality  in 

f  the  tones  of  individual  instruments  with  conical  tubes.  This  opens  rather  a  wide 
field  for  research,  since  the  quality  of  tone  is  altered  in  many  ways  by  the  style  of 
blowing,  and  even  on  the  same  instrument  the  different  parts  of  the  scale,  when 
they  require  the  opening  of  side  holes,  shew  considerable  differences  in  quality. 
On  wooden  wind  instruments  these  differences  are  striking.  The  opening  of  side 
holes  is  by  no  means  a  complete  substitute  for  shortening  the  tube,  and  the  reflec- 
tion of  the  waves  of  sound  at  the  points  of  opening  is  not  the  same  as  at  the  free 
open  end  of  the  tube.  The  upper  partials  of  compound  tones  produced  by  a  tube 
limited  by  an  open  side  hole,  must  certainly  be  in  general  materially  deficient  in 
harmonic  purity,  and  this  will  also  have  a  marked  influence  on  their  resonance.** 

*  [Bat   see  footnote    f    p.   95(2'.~  TVan^-  p.  89,  1.  2,  but  was  cancelled  in    the    4th 

laUyr^  Gennan  edition. — TraiMlaJUiT:\ 

f  [A  line  has  been  here  cancelled  in  the  1  See  Appendix  VIL 

translation  which  had  been  accidentally  left  |  [But  see  note  *  p.  996.— Tmnslo/or/ 

standing  in  the  Grerman,  as  it  refers  to  a  re-  ^*  [The  theory  of  side  holes  is  ezoessiveljr 

mark  on  the  passage  which  formerly  followed  complicated  and  has  not  been  as  yet  worked 

Digitized  by  V^OOQIC 


CHAP.  Y.  7-  yOWEL  QUALITIES  OF  TONE.  103 

7.  Vowel  Qualities  of  Totie. 

We  have  hitherto  discussed  cases  of  resonance,  generated  in  such  air  chambers 
as  were  capable  of  reinforcing  the  prime  tone  principally,  but  also  a  certain 
(number  of  the  harmonic  upper  partial  tones  of  the  compound  tone  produced.    The 
case,  however,  may  also  occur  in  which  the  lowest  tone  of  the  resonance  chamber 
applied  does  not  correspond  with  the  prime,  but  only  with  some  one  of  the  upper 
partials  of  the  compound  tone  itself,  and  in  these  cases  we  find,  in  accordance  with 
the  principles  hitherto  developed,  that  the  corresponding  upper  partial  tone  is 
really  more  reinforced  than  the  prime  or  other  partials  by  the  resonance  of  the 
chamber,  and  consequently  predominates  extremely  over  all  the  other  partials  in 
the  series.     The  quahty  of  tone  thus  produced  has  consequently  a  peculiar  cha- 
racter, and  more  or  less  resembles  one  of  the  vowels  of  the  human  voice.     For  the    j 
vowels  of  speech  are  in  reality  tones  produced  by  membranous  tongues  (the  vocal  Hj 
chords),  with  a  resonance  chamber  (the  mouth)  capable  of  altering  in  length,   jf  ' 
width,  and  pitch  of  resonance,  and  hence  capable  also  of  reinforcing  at  different  f 
times  different  partials  of  the  compound  tone  to  which  it  is  applied.* 

In  ocder  to  understand  the  composition  of  vowel  tones,  we  must  in  the  first 
place  bear  in  mind  that  the  source  of  their  sound  lies  in  the  vocal  chords,  and 
that  when  the  voice  is  heard,  these  chords  act  as  membranous  tongues,  and  like 
all  tongues  produce  a  series  of  decidedly  discontinuous  and  sharply  separated 
pulses  of  air,  which,  on  being  represented  as  a  sum  of  simple  vibrations,  must 
consist  of  a  very  large  number  of  them,  and  hence  be  received  by  the  ear  as  a  very 
long  series  of  partials  belonging  to  a  compound  musical  tone.  With  the  assistance  • 
of  resonators  it  is  possible  to  recognise  very  high  partials,  up  to  the  sixteenth, 
when  one  of  the  brighter  vowels  is  sung  by  a  powerful  bass  voice  at  a  low  pitch, 
and,  in  the  case  of  a  strained  forte  in  the  upper  notes  of  any  human  voice,  we  can 
hear,  more  clearly  than  on  any  other  musical  instrument,  those  high  upper  partials  V 
that  belong  to  the  middle  of  the  four-times  accented  Octave  (the  highest  on 
modem  pianofortes,  see  note,  p.  i8c2),  and  these  high  tones  have  a  peculiar  relation 
to  the  ear,  to  be  subsequently  considered.  The  loudness  of  such  upper  partials, 
especially  those  of  highest  pitch,  differs  considerably  in  different  individuals.  For 
catting  bright  voices  it  is  greater  than  for  soft  and  dull  ones.  The  quality  of  tone 
in  cutting  screaming  voices  may  perhaps  be  referred  to  a  want  of  sufficient 
smoothness  or  straightness  in  the  edges  of  the  vocal  chords,  to  enable  them  to 
close  in  a  straight  narrow  slit  without  striking  one  another.  This  circumstance 
would  give  the  larynx  more  the  character  of  striking  tongues,  and  the  latter  have 
a  much  more  cutting  quality  than  the  free  tongues  of  the  normal  vocal  chords. 
Hoarseness  in  voices  may  arise  from  the  glottis  not  entirely  closing  during  the 
vibrations  of  the  vocal  chords.  At  any  rate,  when  alterations  of  this  kind  are 
made  in  artificial  membranous  tongues,  similar  results  ensue.  For  a  strong  and 
yet  soft  quality  of  voice  it  is  necessary  that  the  vocal  chorda  should,  even  when  H 
most  strongly  vibrating,  join  rectilinearly  at  the  moment  of  approach  with  perfect 
tightness,  effectually  closing  the  glottis  for  the  moment,  but  without  overlapping 

oat  Bcientifically.  *  The  general  principles,*  edited  with  additional  letters  by  W.  S.  Broad- 
writes  Mr.  Blaikley,  *  are  not  difiicolt  of  com-  wood,  and  pablished  by  Budall,  Oarte,  A  Co., 
prehension ;  the  difficulty  is  to  determine  qaan-  makers  of  his  flutes,  tiee  also  Victor  Mahillon, 
titatively  the  values  in  each  p|articular  case/  6tude  sur  le  doigti  de  la  FlAte  Boehm^  1882, 
The  paper  by  Schafhautl  (writing  under  the  and  a  paper  by  M.  Aristide  Cavaill6-Goll,  in 
name  of  Pellisov),  'Theorie  gedeckter  oylin-  I/'&^  Afttf icaZ  for  1 1  Jan.  1883 —Tran«2ator.] 
drischer  und  conischer  Pfeifen  und  der  Quer-  *  The  theory  of  vowel  tones' was  first  enun 
lldten,*  Schweiger,  Joum,  Ixviii.  1833,  is  dis-  elated  by  Wheatstone  in  a  criticism,  unfortu- 
figured  by  misprints  so  that  the  formula  are  nately  little  known,  on  Willises  experiments, 
unintelligible,  and  the  theory  is  also  extremely  The  latter  are  described  in  the  Transactions 
hazardous.  JBut  they  are  the  only  papers  I  of  the  Cambridqe  Philosophical  Society,  vol. 
have  found,  and  are  referred  to  by  Theobald  iii.  p.  231,  and  Poggendorff^s  Annaleyi  der 
.  Boehm,  Veber  den  Fldtenbau,  Mains,  1847.  Physik,  vol.  xxiv.  p.  397.  Wheatstone's  re- 
An  English  version  of  this,  by  himself,  made  port  upon  them  is  contained  in  the  London 
ior  Mr.  Budall  in    1847,  b&s  recently   been  and  }yestmin8i4^  lieview  for  October  1S37. 

Digitized  by  V^OOQ IC 


I04  VOWEL  QUALITIES  OP  TONE.  pabt  i. 

or  striking  against  each  other.  If  they  do  not  close  perfectly,  the  stream  of  air 
ivill  not  be  completely  interrupted,  and  the  tone  cannot  be  powerful.  If  they 
overlap,  the  tone  must  be  cutting,  as  before  remarked,  as  those  arising  from, 
striking  tongues.  On  examining  the  vocal  chords  in  action  by  means  of  a 
laryngoscope,  it  is  marvellous  to  observe  the  accuracy  with  which  they  close  even 
when  making  vibrations  occupying  nearly  the  entire  breadth  of  the  chords  them- 
selves.* 

There  is  also  a  certain  difference  in  the  way  of  putting  on  the  voice  in  speak- 
ing and  in  singing,  which  gives  the  speaking  voice  a  much  more  cutting  quality 
of  tone,  especially  in  the  open  vowels,  and  occasions  a  sensation  of  much  greater 
pressure  in  the  larynx.  I  suspect  that  in  speaking  the  vocal  chords  act  as  striking 
tongues.t 

When  the  mucous  membrane  of  the  larynx  is  affected  with  catarrh,  the 

f  laryngoscope  sometimes  shews  little  flakes  of  mucus  in  the  glottis.  When  these 
are  too  great  they  disturb  the  motion  of  the  vibrating  chords  and  make  them  irre- 
gular, causing  the  tone  to  become  unequal,  jarring,  or  hoarse.  It  is,  however,  re- 
markable what  comparatively  large  flakes  of  mucus  may  lie  in  the  glottis  withoujt 
producing  a  very  striking  deterioration  in  the  quality  of  tone. 

It  has  already  been  mentioned  that  it  is  generally  more  difficult  for  the  un- 
assisted ear  to  recognise  the  upper  partials  in  the  human  voice,  than  in  the  tones 
of  musical  instruments.  Besonators  are  more  necessary  for  this  examination 
than  for  the  analysis  of  any  other  kind  of  musical  tone.  The  upper  partials  of  the 
human  voice  have  nevertheless  been  heard  at  times  by  attentive  observers.  Bameau 
had  heard  them  at  the  beginning  of  last  century.  And  at  a  later  period  8eiler  of 
Leipzig  relates  that  while  listening  to  the  chant  of  the  watchman  during  a  sleepless 
night,  he  occasionally  heard  at  first,  when  the  watchman  was  at  a  distance,  the 
Twelfth  of  the  melody,  and  afterwards  the  prime  tone.    The  reason  of  this  difiiculty 

%  is  most  probably  that  we  have  all  our  lives  remarked  and  observed  the  tones  of 
the  human  voice  more  than  any  other,  and  always  with  the  sole  object  of  grasping 
it  as  a  whole  and  obtaining  a  clear  knowledge  and  perception  of  its  manifold  changes 
of  quality. 

We  may  certainly  assume  that  in  the  tones  of  the  human  larynx,  as  in  all 
other  reed  instruments,  the  upper  partial  tones  would  decrease  in  force  as  they 
increase  in  pitch,  if  they  could  be  observed  without  the  resonance  of  the  cavity  of 
the  mouth.  In  reality  they  satisfy  this  assumption  tolerably  well,  for  those  vowels 
iriiich  are  spoken  with  a  wide  funnel-shaped  cavity  of  the  mouth,  as  A  [a  in  art],  or 
jL[ain  bat  lengthened,  which  is  nearly  the  same  as  a  in  bare].  But  this  relation  is 
materially  altered  by  the  resonance  which  takes  place  in  the  cavity  of  the  mouth. 
The  more  this  cavity  is  narrowed,  either  by  the  lips  or  the  tongue,  the  more  dis- 
tinctly marked  is  its  resonance  for  tones  of  determinate  pitch,  and  the  more  there- 
fore does  this  resonance  reinforce  those  partials  in  the  compound  tone  produced  by 

%  the  vocal  chords,  which  approach  the  favoured  pitch,  and  the  more,  on  the  contrary, 
win  the  others  be  damped.  Hence  on  investigating  the  compound  tones  of  the 
human  voice  by  means  of  resonators,  we  find  pretty  uniformly  that  the  first  six  to 
eight  partials  are  clearly  perceptible,  but  with  very  different  degrees  of  force  accord- 
ing to  the  different  forms  of  the  cavity  of  the  mouth,  sometimes  screaming  loudly 
into  the  ear,  at  others  scarcely  audible. 

Under  these  circumstances  the  investigation  of  the  resonance  of  the  cavity  of 
the  mouth  is  of  great  importance.  The  easiest  and  surest  method  of  finding  the 
tones  to  which  the  air  in  the  oral  cavity  is  tuned  for  the  different  shapes  it  assumes 

*  [Probably  these  observations  were  made  f  [The  German  habit  of 

on  the  *  upper  thick  *  register,  because  the  vowels  with  the  '  check  *  or  Arabic 

chords  are  then  more  visible.    It  is  evident  which  is  very  marked,  and  instantly  charac- 

that  these  theories  do  not  apply  to  the  lower  terises  his  nationality,  is  probably  what    is 

thick,  upper  thin,  and  small  registers,  and  here  alluded  to,  as  occasioning  a  sensation  of 

scarcely  to  the  lower  thin,  as  described  above,  much  greater  pressure.    This  does  not  apply 

footnote  p.  loic— Translator.]  in  the  least  to  English  speakers.— -Traf»2ator^ 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


CHAP.  V.   7* 


VOWEL  QUALITIES  OF  TONE. 


"5 


in  the  production  of  vowels,  is  that  which  is  used  for  glass  bottles  and  other  spaces 
fiUed  with  air.  That  is,  tuning-forks  of  different  pitches  have  to  be  struck  and 
held  before  the  opening  of  the  air  chamber — ^in  the  present  case  the  open  mouth 
— and  the  louder  the  proper  tone  of  the  fork  is  heard,  the  nearer  does  it  corre- 
spond with  one  of  the  proper  tones  of  the  included  mass  of  air.*  Since  the  shape 
of  the  oral  cavity  can  be  altered  at  pleasure,  it  can  always  be  made  to  suit  the 
tone  of  any  given  tuning-fork,  and  we  thus  easily  discover  what  shape  the  mouih 
must  assume  for  its  included,  mass  of  air  to  be  tuned  to  a  determinate  pitch. 

Having  a  series  of  tuning-forks  at  command,  I  was  thus  able  to  obtain  the 
following  results : — 

The  pitch  of  strongest  resonance  of  the  oral  cavity  depends  solely  upon  the 
vowel  for  pronouncing  which  the  mouth  has  been  arranged,  and  alters  considerably 
for  even  slight  alterations  in  the  vowel  quality,  such,  for  example,  as  occur  in  the 
different  dialects  of  the  same  language.  On  the  other  hand,  the  proper  tones  of  f 
the  cavity  of  the  mouth  are  nearly  independent  of  age  and  sex.  I  have  in  general 
fonnd  the  same  resonances  in  men,  women,  and  children.  The  want  of  space  in 
the  oral  cavity  of  women  and  children  can  be  easily  replaced  by  a  great  closure  of  its 
opening,  which  will  make  the  resonance  as  deep  as  in  the  larger  oral  cavities  of  men.f 

The  vowels  can  be  arranged  in  three  series,  according  to  the  position  of  the 
parts  of  the  mouth,  which  may  be  written  thus,  in  accordance  with  Du  Bois- 
Beymond  the  elder  J : — 

E      I 


tJ 

U 

The  vowel  A  [a  in  father,  or  Scotch  a  in  man\  forms  the  common  origin  of 
all  three  series.    With  this  vowel  corresponds  a  funnel-shaped  resonance  cavity,  1 


*  [See  note  *  p.  876,  on  determining  violin 
resonaaoe.  One  diffioodty  in  the  ease  of  the 
mouth  is  that  there  is  a  constant  tendency  to 
varythe  shape  of  the  oral  cavity.  Another,  as 
shewn  at  the  end  of  the  note  cited,  is  that 
the  same  irregular  cavity,  such  as  that  of  the 
znouth,  often  more  or  less  reinforces  a  large 
numbor  of  different  tones.  As  it  was  impor- 
tant for  my  phonetic  researches,  I  have  niade 
many  attempts  to  determine  mv  own  vowel 
resonances,  but  have  hitherto  failed  in  all  my 
attempts.  —Translaior.} 

t  [Easily  tried  by  more  or  less  covering 
the  top  of  a  tumbler  with  the  hand,  till  it 
resounds  to  any  fork  from  d  to  d"  or  higher. 
—Translaior.] 

t  Norddeuische  ZeitBchrift,  edited  by  de 
la  Motte  Fouqu6,  181 2.  Kadmus  oder  cUlge- 
fneine  Alphabetik,  von  F.  H.  du  Bois-Beymond, 
Berlin,  1862,  p.  152.  [This  is  the  arrange- 
ment usually  adopted.  But  in  1867  Mr. 
Melville  Bell,  an  orthoepical  teacher  of  many 
years*  standing,  who  had  been  led  profession- 
aUy  to  pay  great  attention  to  the  shapes  of  the 
mouth  necessary  to  produce  certain  sounds,  in 
his  Visible  Speech;  the  Science  of  Universal 
Alphabetics  (London:  Simpkin,  Marshall  & 
Co.,  4to.,  pp.  X.  126,  with  sixteen  lithographic 
tables),  proposed  a  more  elaborate  method  of 
classifying  vowels  by  the  shape  of  the  mouth. 
He  conmienced  with  9  positions  of  the  tongue, 
consisting  of  3  in  which  the  middle,  or  as  he 
terms  it,  *  front '  of  the  tongue  was  raised, 
highest  for  ea  in  seat^  not  so  l]dgh  for  a  in  sate, 
and  lowest  for  a  in  sat\  3  others  in  which  the 
p'  back,  instead  of  the  middle,  of  the  tongue 


was  raised,  highest  for  00  in  snood,  lower  for  o 
in  node,  and  lowest  for  aw  in  gna/wed  (noiie  of 
which  three  are  determined  by  the  position  of 
the  tongue  alone),  and  3  intermediate  positimis, 
where  the  whole  tongue  is  raised  almost  evenly 
at  three  different  elevations.  These  9  Ungual 
positions  might  be  accompanied  with  the 
ordinary  or  with  increased  distension  of  the 
pharynx,  giving  9  primary  and  9  *wide' 
vowels.  And  each  of  the  18  vowels,  thus 
produced,  could  be  *  rounded,*  that  is,  modified 
by  shading  the  mouth  in  various  degrees  with 
the  lips.  He  thus  obtains  36  distinct  vowel 
cavities,  among  which  almost  all  those  used 
for  vowel  qualities  in  different  nations  may  be 
placed.  Subsequent  research  has  shewn  how 
to  extend  this  arrangement  materially.  See  ^ 
my  Early  English  Pronunciation,  part  iv., 
1874,  p.  1279.  Also  see  generally  my  Pro- 
nunciationfor  Singers  (Curwen,  1877,  pp.  246) 
and  Speech  in  Song  (NoveUo,  1878,  pp.  140). 
German  vowels  differ  materially  in  qualit7 
from  the  English,  and  consequently  complete 
agreement  between  Prof.  Helmholtz's  obser- 
vations and  those  of  any  Englishman,  who 
repeats  his  experiments,  must  not  be  expected. 
I  have  consequently  thought  it  better  in  this 
place  to  leave  his  German  notation  untrans- 
lated, and  merely  subjoin  in  parentheses  the 
nearest  English  sounds.  For  the  table  in  the 
text  we  may  assume  A  to  »  a  in  faXher,  or  else 
Scotch  a  in  man  (different  sounds),  E  to  *-  erin 
there,  I  to  »  i  in  machine,  O  to  »  o  in  more,  U 
to  -*  tt  in  sure ;  and  0  to  »  sii  in  French  peu 
or  else  in  peuple  (different  sounds),  and  tf  to 
«  tt  in  French  pu,— Translator.] 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


xo6  VOWEL  QUALITIES  OF  TONE.  pabt  i, 

enlargmg  with  tolerable  uniformity  from  the  larynx  to  the  lips.  For  the  vowels  of 
the  lower  series,  0  [o  in  more]  and  U  [oo  in  poor],  the  opening  of  the  mouth  is 
contracted  by  means  of  the  Hps,  more  for  U  than  for  0,  while  the  cavity  is  enlarged 
as  much  as  possible  by  depression  of  the  tongue,  so  that  on  the  whole  it  becomes 
like  a  bottle  without  a  neck,  with  rather  a  narrow  mouth,  and  a  single  unbroken 
cavity.*  The  pitch  of  such  a  bottle-shaped  chamber  is  lower  the  larger  its  cavity 
and  the  narrower  its  mouth.  Usually  only  one  upper  partial  with  strong  resonance 
can  be  clearly  recognised ;  when  other  proper  tones  exist  they  are  comparatively 
very  high,  or  have  only  weak  resonance.  In  conformity  with  these  results,  obtained 
with  glass  bottles,  we  find  that  for  a  very  deep  hollow  U  [oo  in  poor  nearly],  where 
the  oral  cavity  is  widest  and  the  mouth  narrowest,  the  resonance  is  deepest  and 
answers  to  the  unaccented/.  On  passing  from  U  to  0  [o  in  more  nearly]  the 
resonance  gradually  rises  ;  and  for  a  full,  ringing,  pure  0  the  pitch  is  6^«     The 

m  position  of  the  mouth  for  0  is  peculiarly  favourable  for  resonance,  the  opening  of  the 
mouth  being  neither  too  large  nor  too  small,  and  the  internal  cavity  sufficiently 
spacious.  Hence  if  a  b'\}  tuning-fork  be  struck  and  held  before  the  mouth  while  0 
is  gently  uttered,  or  the  O-position  merely  assumed  without  really  speaking,  the  tone 
of  the  fork  will  resound  so  fully  and  loudly  that  a  large  audience  can  hear  it.  The 
usual  a'  tuning-fork  of  musicians  may  also  be  used  for  this  purpose,  but  then  it  will  be 
necessary  to  make  a  somewhat  duller  O,  if  w^e  wish  to  bring  out  the  full  resonance. 
On  gradually  bringing  the  shape  of  the  mouth  from  the  position  proper  to  0, 
through  those  due  to  0*  [nearly  o  in  cot,  with  rather  more  of  the  0  sound],  and  A** 
[nearly  au  in  caught,  with  rather  more  of  the  A  sound]  into  that  for  A  [Scotch  a 
in  man,  with  rather  more  of  an  0  quahty  in  it  than  English  a  in  father],  the 
resonance  gradually  rises  an  Octave,  and  reaches  h'*\}.  This  tone  corresponds  with 
the  North  German  A ;  the  somewhat  brighter  A  [a  in  father]  of  the  English  and 
Itahans  rises  up  to  d'*\  or  a  major  Third  higher.     It  is  particularly  remarkable  what 

„  little  differences  in  pitch  correspond  to  very  sensible  varieties  of  vowel  quality  in 
the  neighbourhood  of  A  ;  and  I  should  therefore  recommend  philologists  who  wish 
to  define  the  vowels  of  different  languages  to  fix  them  by  the  pitch  of  loudest 
resonance.t 

For  the  vowels  already  mentioned  I  have  not  been  able  to  detect  any  second 
proper  tone,  and  the  analogy  of  the  phenomena  presented  by  ai'tificial  resonance 
chambers  of  similar  shapes  would  hardly  lead  us  to  expect  any  of  sensible  loudness. 

*  [This  depressed  position  of  the  tongne  able  to  discriminate  vowel  sounds,  is  frequently 
answers  better  for  English  aw  in  saw  than  for  not  acute  for  differences  of  pitch.  The  deter- 
either  o  in  more  or  oo  in  poor.  For  the  o  the  mination  of  the  pitch  even  under  favour- 
tongue  is  slightly  more  raised,  especially  at  the  able  circumstances  is  not  easy,  especially,  as  it 
back,  while  for  English  oo  the  back  of  the  will  be  seen,  for  the  higher  pitches.  Without 
tongue  is  almost  as  high  as  for  k,  and  greatly  mechanical  appliances  even  good  ears  are 
impedes  the  oral  cavity.  If,  however,  the  deceived  in  the  Octave.  The  differences  of 
longue  be  kept  in  the  position  for  aw  by  sound-  pitch  noted  by  Helmholtz,  Donders,  Merkel, 
^  ing  this  vowel,  and,  while  sounding  it  steadily,  and  Koenig,  as  given  on  p.  I09d,  probably  point 
'<  the  lips  be  gradually  contracted,  the  sound  to  fundamental  differences  of  pronunciation, 
will  be  found  to  pass  through  certain  obscure  and  shew  the  desirability  of  a  very  extensive 
qualities  of  tone  till  it  suddenly  comes  out  series  of  experiments  being  carried  out  with 
clearly  as  a  sound  a  little  more  like  aw  than  o  special  apparatus,  by  an  operator  with  an 
in  more  (really  the  Danish  aa),  and  then  again  extremely  acute  musical  ear,  on  speakers  of 
passing  through  other  obscure  phases,  comes  various  nationalities  and  also  on  various 
out  again  clearly  as  a  deep  sound,  not  so  bright  speakers  of  the  same  nationaUty.  Chreat  difli- 
as  our  00  in  poor,  but  more  resembling  the  culty  will  even  then  be  experienced  on  account 
Swedish  o  to  which  it  will  reach  if  the  tongue  of  the  variability  of  the  same  speaker  in  his 
be  slightly  raised  into  the  A  position.  It  is  vowel  quality  for  differences  of  pitch  and 
necessary  to  bear  these  facts  in  mind  when  expression,  the  want  of  habit  to  maintain  the 
following  the  text,  where  U  is  only  almost,  not  position  of  the  mouth  unmoved  for  a  sufficient 
quite  =  oo  in  poor,  which  is  the  long  sound  of  u  length  of  time  to  complete  an  observation 
in  pull^  and  is  duller  than  oo  in  pool  or  French  satisfactorily,  and,  worst  of  all,  the  involuntary 
ou  in  poule.— Translator.]  tendency  of  the  organs  to  accommodate  them- 

f  [Great  difficulties  lie  in  the  way  of  carry-  selves  to  the  pitch  of  the  fork  presented.  Com- 
ing out  this  recommendation.     The  ear  of  pare  note  *  p.  105c. — Translator.] 


philologists  and  even  of  those  who  are  readily 


Digitized  by 


Google 


CHAP.  V.  7.  VOWEL  QUALITIES  OF  TONK  107 

Experiments  hereafter  desoribed  shew  that  the  resonance  of  this  single  tone  is 
sufficient  to  characterise  the  vowels  above  mentioned. 

The  second  series  of  vowels  consists  of  A,  A,  E,  I.  The  lips  are  drawn  so  far 
apart  that  they  no  longer  contract  the  issuing  stream  of  air,  but  a  firesh  constric- 
tion is  formed  between  the  front  (middle)  parts  of  the  tongue  and  the  hard  palate, 
the  space  immediately  above  the  larynx  being  widened  by  depressing  the  root  of 
the  tongue,  and  hence  causing  the  larynx  to  rise  simultaneously.  The  form  of  the 
oral  cavity  consequently  resembles  a  bottle  with  a  narrow  neck.  The  belly  of  the 
bottle  is  behind,  in  the  pharynx,  and  its  neck  is  the  narrow  passage  between  the 
upper  surface  of  the  tongue  and  the  hard  palate.  In  the  above  series  of  letters, 
A,  E,  I,  these  changes  increase  until  for  I  the  internal  cavity  of  the  bottle  is  greatest 
and  the  neck  narrowest.  For  A  [the  broadest  French  ij  broader  than  e  in  therCj 
and  nearly  as  broad  as  a  in  bat  lengthened,  with  which  the  name  of  their  city  is 
pronounced  by  the  natives  of  Bath],  the  whole  channel  is,  however,  tolerably  wide,  IF 
80  that  it  is  quite  easy  to  see  down  to  the  larynx  when  the  laryngoscope  is  used. 
Indeed  this  vowel  gives  the  very  best  position  of  the  mouth  for  the  application  of 
this  instrument,  because  the  root  of  the  tongue,  which  impedes  the  view  when  A 
is  uttered,  is  depressed,  and  the  observer  can  see  over  and  past  it. 

When  a  bottle  with  a  long  narrow  neck  is  used  as  a  resonance  chamber,  two 
simple  tones  are  readily  discovered,  of  which  one  can  be  regarded  as  the  proper 
tone  of  the  belly,  and  the  other  as  that  of  the  neck  of  the  bottle.  Of  course  the 
air  in  the  belly  cannot  vibrate  quite  independently  of  that  in  the  neck,  and  both 
proper  tones  in  question  must  consequently  be  different,  and  indeed  somewhat 
deeper  than  they  would  be  if  belly  and  neck  were  separate  and  had  their  resonance 
examined  independently.  The  neck  is  approximately  a  short  pipe  open  at  both 
ends.  To  be  sure,  its  inner  end  debouches  into  the  cavity  of  the  bottle  instead  0£ 
the  open  air,  but  if  the  neck  is  very  narrow,  and  the  belly  of  the  bottle  very  wide, 
the  latter  may  be  looked  upon  in  some  respect  as  an  open  space  with  regard  to  the  % 
vibrations  of  the  air  inclosed  in  the  neck.  These  conditions  are  best  satisfied  for 
I,  in  which  the  length  of  the  channel  between  tongue  and  palate,  measured  from 
the  upper  teeth  to  the  back  edge  of  the  bony  palate,  is  about  6  centimetres  [2*36 
inches].  An  open  pipe  of  this  length  when  blown  would  give  e""^  while  the 
observations  made  for  determining  the  tone  of  loudest  resonance  for  I  gives  nearly 
d'^^',  which  is  as  close  an  agreement  as  could  possibly  have  been  expected  in  such 
an  irregularly  shaped  pipe  as  that  formed  by  the  tongue  and  palate. 

In  accordance  with  these  experiments  the  vowels  A,  E,  I  have  each  a  higher 
and  a  deeper  resonance  tone.  The  higher  tones  continue  the  ascending  series  of 
the  proper  tones  of  the  vowels  U,  0,  A.  By  means  of  tuning-forks  I  found  for  A 
a  tone  between  g'^'  and  a'"\},  and  for  E  the  tone  &'''[>•  ^  ^^  ^o  ^^^^  suitable  for 
I,  but  by  means  of  the  whistling  noise  of  the  air,  to  be  considered  presently 
Cp,  108ft),  its  proper  tone  was  determined  with  tolerable  exactness  to  be  d'"'. 

The  deeper  proper  tones  which  are  due  to  the  back  part  of  the  oral  cavity  are  % 
rather  more  difficult  to  discover.  Tuning-forks  may  be  used,  but  the  resonance  is 
comparatively  weak,  because  it  must  be  conducted  through  the  long  narrow  neck 
of  the  air  chamber.  It  must  further  be  remembered  that  this  resonance  only 
occurs  during  the  time  that  the  corresponding  vowel  is  gently  whispered,  and  dis- 
appears as  soon  as  the  whisper  ceases,  because  the  form  of  the  chamber  on  which 
the  resonance  depends  then  immediately  changes.  The  tuning-forks  after  being 
struck  must  be  brought  as  close  as  possible  to  the  opening  of  the  air  chamber 
which  lies  behind  the  upper  teeth.  By  this  means  I  found  d"  for  A  and/  for  E. 
For  I,  direct  observation  with  tuning-forks  was  not  possible ;  but  from  the  upper 
partial  tones,  I  conclude  that  its  proper  tone  is  as  deep  as  that  of  U,  or  near/. 
Hence,  when  we  pass  from  A  to  I,  these  deeper  proper  tones  of  the  oral  cavity  sink, 
and  the  higher  ones  rise  in  pitch.* 

♦  [Mr.  Graham  Bell,  the  inventor  of  the  mentioned  (p.  I05<Z,  note),  was  in  the  habit  of 
Telephone,  son  of  the  Mr.  Melville  Bell  already      bringing  out  this  fact  by  placing  his  mouth  in 

Digitized  by  V^OOQIC 


to8  VOWEL  QUALITIES  OF  TONE.  pabt  i. 

For  the  third  series  of  vowels  from  A  through  0  [French  eu  in  peu,  or  the 
deeper  eu  in  peuple],  towards  tJ  [French  u  in  pu,  which  is  rather  deeper  than  the 
German  sound],  we  have  the  same  internal  positions  of  the  mouth  as  in  the  last- 
named  series  of  vowels.  For  tl  the  mouth  is  placed  in  nearly  the  same  position 
as  for  a  vowel  lying  between  E  and  I,  and  for  0  as  for  an  E  which  inclines  towards 
A.  In  addition  to  the  constriction  between  the  tongue  and  palate  as  in  the  second 
series,  we  have  also  a  constriction  of  the  lips,  which  are  made  into  a  sort  of  tube, 
forming  a  front  prolongation  of  that  made  by  the  tongue  and  palate.  The  air 
chamber  of  the  mouth,  therefore,  in  this  case  also  resembles  a  bottle  with  a  neck, 
but  the  neck  is  longer  than  for  the  second  series  of  vowels.  For  I  the  neck  was 
6  centimetres  (2*36  inches)  long,  for  t7,  measured  from  the  front  edge  of  the  upper 
teeth  to  the  commencement  of  the  soft  palate,  it  is  8  centimetres  (3*15  inches). 
The  pitch  of  the  higher  proper  tone  corresponding  to  the  resonance  of  the  neck 

%  must  be,  therefore,  about  a  Fourth  deeper  than  for  I.  If  both  ends  were  free,  a  pipe 
of  this  length  would  give  b'",  according  to  the  usual  calculation.  In  reahty  it 
resounded  for  a  fork  lying  between  ^'"  and  a"^,  a  divergence  similar  to  that 
found  for  I,  and  also  probably  attributable  to  the  back  end  of  the  tube  debouching 
into  a  wider  but  not  quite  open  space.  The  resonance  of  the  back  space  has  to  be 
observed  in  the  same  way  as  for  the  I  series.  For  0  it  is  /,  the  same  as  for  E, 
and  for  O  it  is/,  the  same  as  for  I. 

The  fact  that  the  cavity  of  the  mouth  for  different  vowels  is  tuned  to  different 
pitches  was  first  discovered  by  Bonders,*  not  with  the  help  of  tuning-forks,  but  by 
the  whistling  noise  produced  in  the  mouth  by  whispering.  The  cavity  of  the 
mouth  thus  reinforces  by  its  resonance  the  corresponding  tones  of  the  windrush, 
which  are  produced  partly  in  the  contracted  glottis,t  and  partly  in  the  forward 
contracted  passages  of  the  mouth.  In  this  way  it  is  not  usual  to  obtain  a  complete 
musical  tone  ;  this  only  happens,  without  sensible  change  of  the  vowel,  for  tJ  and 

f  U,  when  a  real  whistle  is  produced.  This,  however,  would  be  a  fault  in  speaking. 
We  have  rather  only  such  a  degree  of  reinforcement  of  the  noise  of  the  air  as 
occurs  in  an  organ  pipe,  which  does  not  speak  well,  either  from  a  badly-constructed 
lip  or  an  insufficient  pressure  of  wind.  A  noise  of  this  kind,  although  not  brought 
up  to  being  a  complete  musical  tone,  has  nevertheless  a  tolerably  determinate 
pitch,  which  can  be  estimated  by  a  practised  ear.  But,  as  in  all  cases  where  tones 
of  very  different  qualities  have  to  be  compared,  it  is  easy  to  make  a  mistake  in  the 
Octave.    However,  after  some  of  the  important  pitches  have  been  determined  by 

the  required  positions  and  then  tapping  against  Chr.  Hellwag,  De  FormaUone  Loqudae  DisM,^ 

a  finger  placed  just  in  front  of  the  upper  teeth,  Tubingtiet    17 10. — Fldrcke,     Neus     Berlitur 

for  the  higher  resonance,  and  placed  against  Monatsschrift,  Sept.  1803,  Feb.   1804. — Olivier 

the  neck,  just  above  the  larynx,  for  the  lower.  Ortho  -  epo  -  graphischea    Elementar  -  Werk^ 

He  obligingly  performed  the  experiment  several  1S04,  part  iii.  p.  21. 

times  privately  before  me,  and  the  successive  t  In  whispering,  the  vocal  chords  are  kept 
alterations  and  differences  in  their  direction  close,  but  the  air  passes  through  a  small 
m  were  striking.  The  tone  was  dull  and  like  triangular  opening  at  the  back  part  of  the 
a  wood  hannonica.  Considerable  dexterity  glottis  between  the  arytenoid  cartilages.  [Ac- 
seemed  necessary  to  produce  the  effect,  and  I  cording  to  Gzermak  (SiUungiberichte^  Wiener 
could  not  succeed  in  doing  so.  He  carried  out  Akad.,  Math.-Naturw.  01.  April  29,  1858, 
the  experiment  much  further  than  is  suggested  p.  576)  the  vocal  chords  as  seen  through  tiie 
in  the  text,  embracing  the  whole  nine  positions  laryngoscope  are  not  quite  close  for  whisper, 
of  the  tongue  in  his  father's  vowel  scheme,  but  are  nicked  in  the  middle.  Merkel  (Die 
and  obtaining  a  double  resonance  in  each  case.  Funktionen  des  menschlichen  Schlund-  und 
This  fact  is  stated,  and  the  various  vowel  Kehlkopfea,  .  .  .  nach  eigenen  pharyngo-  und 
theories  appreciated  in  Mr.  Graham  Bell's  laryngoakopischen  Untersuchungent  Leipzig, 
paper  on  *  Vowel  Theories  *  read  before  the  1862,  p.  77)  distinguishes  two  kinds  of  whisper- 
American  National  Academy  of  Arts  and  ing:  (i)  the  loud,  in  which  the  opening  between 
Sciences,  April  15,  1879,  &nd  printed  in  the  the  chords  is  from  |  to  )  of  a  line  wide,  pro- 
Ameriean  Journal  of  Otology ,  vol.  i.  July  duoing  no  resonant  viorations,  and  that  between 
1879. — Translator.]  the  arytenoids  is  somewhat  wider;  (2)  the 
*  Arehiv  fUr  die  Holldndischen  BeitrOge  gentle,  in  which  the  vowel  is  commenced  as  in 
fUr  Natur-  tmd  HeUkunde  von  Bonders  und  loud  speaking,  with  dosed  glottis,  and,  after  it 
Berlin,  vol.  i.  p.  157.  Older  incomplete  obser-  has  begun,  the  back  part  of  the  glottis  is 
vations  of  the  same  circumstance  in  Samuel  opened,  while  the  chords  remain  close  and 
Beyher's    Matliesis    Mosaica,    Kiel,   161 9.^  motionless.— TramlatorJ] 

Digitized  by  VjOOQlC 


CHAP.  V.    7. 


VOWEL  QUALITIES  OF  TONE. 


109 


tuning-forks,  and  others,  as  tJ  and  0,  by  allowing  the  whisper  to  pass  into  a 
regular  whistle,  the  rest  are  easily  determined  by  arranging  them  in  a  melodio 
progression  with  the  first.    Thus  the  series : — 


aearA 

A 

B 

I 

[a  in  father] 

[a  in  ma€] 

\t  in  ikwe\ 

[t  in  machine] 

forms  an  ascending  minor  chord  of  g  in  the  second  Inversion  J,  [with  the  Fifth  in 
the  bass,]  and  can  be  readily  compared  with  the  same  melodic  progression  on  the 
pianoforte.  I  was  able  to  determine  the  pitch  for  clear  A,  A,  and  E  by  tuning- 
forks,  and  hence  to  fix  that  for  I  also.* 


*  The  statements  of  Bonders  differ  slightly 
from  mine,  partly  because  they  have  reference 
to  Dutch  pronunciation,  while  mine  refer  to  the 
North   German  vowels;    and  partly  because 


Bonders,  not  having  been  assisted  by  tuning-  «r 
forks,  was  not  always  able  to  determine  with 
certainty  to  what  Octave  the  noises  he  heard 
should  be  assigned. 


Pitch  accord. 

Fitch  aooord- 

Vowel 

inirto 

Inifto 

Dondora 

Helmholts 

U 

r 

/ 

0 

d' 

6'b 

A 

6'b 

6"b 

0 

fl'? 

c'"J 

u 

a" 

fif^'toa'^b 

E 

c'"J 

6'"b 

I 

/'" 

d"" 

[The  extreme  divergence  of  results  obtained 
by  different  investigators  shews  the  inherent 
^^oultias  of  the  determination,  which  (as 
already  indicated)  arise  partly  from  different 
values  attributed  to  the  vowels,  partly  from  the 
difficulty  of  retaining  the  form  of  the  mouth 
steadily  for  a  suflcient  time,  partly  from  the 
wide  range  of  tones  which  the  same  cavity  of 
the  mouth  will  more  or  less  reinforce,  partly 
from  the  dif&culty  of  judging  of  absolute  pitch 
in  general,  and  especially  of  the  absolute  pitch 
of  a  scarcely  musical  whisper,  and  other  causes. 
In  C.  L.  MerkePs  Physioloaie  der  mensch- 
lichen  Sprache  (Leipzig,  1866),  p.  47,  a  table  is 
given  of  the  results  of  Beyher,  Hellwag, 
Fl&rcke,  and  Bonders  (the  latter  differing  ma- 
terially from  that  just  given  by  Prof.  Helm- 
hoHz),  and  on  MerkePs  p.  109,  he  adds  his  last 


results.  These  are  reproduced  in  the  following 
table  with  the  notes,  and  their  pitch  to  the  f 
nearest  vibration,  taking  a'  440,  and  supposing 
equal  temperament.  To  these  I  add  the  re* 
suits  of  Bonders,  as  just  given,  and  of  Helm- 
holtz,  both  with  pitches  similarly  assumed. 
Koenig  {Comptee  HendtUy  April  25,  1870)  also 
gives  his  pitches  with  exact  numbers,  reckoEed 
as  Octaves  of  the  7th  harmonic  of  c'  256,  and 
hence  called  6)>,  although  they  are  nearer  the 
a  of  this  standard.  Reference  should  also  be 
made  to  Br.  Eoenig*s  paper  on  '  Manometrio 
Flames*  translated  in  the  Philosophical  Maga- 
tine,  1873,  vol.  xlv.  pp.  1-18, 105-114.  Lastly, 
Br.  Moritz  Trautmann  (Anglia,  vol.  i.  p.  590) 
very  confidently  gives  results  utterly  different 
from  all  the  above,  which  I  subjoin  with  the 
pitch  as  before.    I  give  the  general  form  of 


TABI.K  OF  YOWRL  BeAONANCKS. 

Obserrtr. 

V 

0 

A 

A         1         B 

I 

V 

0 

I.  Reyher  . 

c  131 

flWi56 

a  990  ) 

c'a62r 

dIiS6 

/'349 

c"sa3 

2.  Hellwag 

cist 

CI139 

aaao 

6247 

0^262 

6b  233 

j;r92o8 

3.  Flttrcke . 

C131 

^196 

c'a6a 

^^392 

d'440 

e"S23 

^^392 

•'SSo 

4,  Bonden     ac- . 
cording       to  i 
Helmholts    .[ 

/'349 

<f  294 

6'b  466 

/"i«;68 

c'TT  1109 

/^i397 

a"  880 

^196? 

+drs^ 

S.  Bonders     ac-x 
cording       to  I 
Merkel          . ) 

y65  J 
t;.7i75 

ei6s 

6047 

c's6a 

/"698 

0*440 

^196 

6.  Helmiioltz. 

6'b  466 

6"b93« 

^'"1568 

6*"  1976 

if"' 2349 

^1568 

cTt  1 109 

Ou,/'349 

+f  587 

Jt-C,349 

V^ 

+/I7S 

■t/'349 

7.  Merkel  .       . 

di47 

0*,yi96 

A\/I320 

^"587 
or  0' 440 

ly^^^^ 

a' 440 

/»370 

A',  6  247 

E',«^659 

or<f  294 

8.KoenSg,     7th 

harmonics     . 

6b  234 

6'b  448 

6''b896 

i,T^J792 

ft'"b3584 

0.  Trautmann   . 

regs 

O\c'"io47 

/'"  1397 

-F? 

EV  1760:/""  2794 

6-1976 

*,iriS68 

I 

O',a"88o 

E',c""2093l 

iO'/i'"i76o| 

Digitized  by  V^OOQIC 


no 


VOWEL  QUALITIES  OP  TONE, 


PABT   I. 


For  U  it  is  also  by  no  means  easy  to  find  the  pitch  of  the  resonance  by  a  fork, 
as  the  smaUness  of  the  opening  makes  the  resonance  weak.  Another  phenomenon 
has  guided  me  in  this  ease.  If  I  sing  the  scale  from  c  upwards,  uttering  the  vowel 
U  for  each  note,  and  taking  care  to  keep  the  quality  of  the  vowel  correct,  and  not 
allowing  it  to  pass  into  0,*  I  feel  the  agitation  of  the  air  in  the  mouth,  and  even 
en  the  drums  of  both  ears,  where  it  excites  a  tickling  sensation,  most  powerfully 
when  the  voice  reaches  /.  As  soon  as  /  is  passed  the  quality  changes,  the  strong^ 
agitation  of  the  air  in  the  mouth  and  the  tickling  in  the  ears  cease.  For  the  note 
/  the  phenomenon  in  this  case  is  the  same  as  if  a  spherical  resonance  chamber 
were  placed  before  a  tongue  of  nearly  the  same  pitch  as  its  proper  tone.  In  this 
case  also  we  have  a  powerful  agitation  of  the  air  within  the  sphere  and  a  sudden 
alteration  of  quality  of  tone,  on  passing  from  a  deeper  pitch  of  the  mass  of  air 
through  that  of  the  tongue  to  a  higher.  The  resonance  of  the  mouth  for  U  is  thus 
^  fixed  at/  with  more  certainty  than  by  means  of  tuning-forks.  But  we  often  meet 
with  a  U  of  higher  resonance,  more  resembling  0,  which  I  will  represent  by  the 
French  Ou.  Its  proper  tone  may  rise  as  high  as  /.f  The  resonance  of  the 
cavity  of  the  mouth  for  different  vowels  may  then  be  expressed  in  notes  as  follows  : 


S      j;     h*0    h'h 


d"   f 


E^*^ 


f  gfe 


'%  9" 


f 


:tr— tr= 


U    Ou 


0 


1      E 


f 


I      ±: 

0    n 


^  The  mode  in  which  the  resonance  of  the  cavity  of  the  mouth  acts  upod  the 
quality  of  the  voice,  is  then  precisely  the  same  as  that  which  we  discovered  to 
exist  for  artificially  constructed  reed  pipes.  All  those  partial  tones  are  reinforced 
which  coincide  with  a  proper  tone  of  the  cavity  of  the  mouth,  or  have  a  pitch 
sufficiently  near  to  that  of  such  a  tone,  while  the  other  partial  tones  will  be  more 
or  less  damped.  The  damping  of  those  partial  tones  which  are  not  strengthened 
is  the  more  striking  the  narrower  the  opening  of  the  mouth,  either  between  the 
lips  as  for  U,  or  between  the  tongue  and  palate  as  for  I  and  IT. 

These  differences  in  the  partial  tones  of  the  different  vowel  sounds  can  be  easily 
and  clearly  recognised  by  means  of  resonators,  at  least  within  the  once  and  twice 
accented  Octaves  [264  to  1056  vib.] .  For  example,  apply  a  6't>  resonator  to  the 
ear,  and  get  a  bass  voice,  that  can  preserve  pitch  well  and  form  its  vowels  with 
purity,  to  sing  the  series  of  vowels  to  one  of  the  harmonic  under  tones  of  6%  such 
as  b\},  e\},  P|>,  G\}i  E\}.    It  will  be  found  that  for  a  pure,  full-toned  0  the  &'(>  of 

f  the  resonator  will  bray  violently  into  the  ear.  The  same  upper  partial  tone  is 
still  very  powerful  for  a  clear  A  and  a  tone  intermediate  between  A  and  0,  but  is 
weaker  for  A,  E,  0,  and  weakest  of  all  for  U  and  I.  It  will  also  be  found  that 
the  resonance  of  0  is  materially  weakened  if  it  is  taken  too  duU,  approaching  U» 


the  vowel  at  the  head  of  each  column,  and 
when  the  writer  distinguishes  different  forms 
I  add  them  immediately  before  the  resonance 
note.  Thus  we  have  Helmholtz*8  Ou  between 
U  and  O ;  MerkePs  O  between  O  and  A,  his 
obscure  A\  K'  and  clear  A',  E' ;  Trautmann*s 
O'  =  Italian  open  O,  and  (as  he  says)  English 
a  in  all  (which  is,  however,  slightly  different), 
O'  ordinary  o  in  Berliner  ohne,  E'  Berlin 
Schnee,  E'  French  pire  (the  same  as  X  ?),  0' 
Berlin  schOn,  French  peu,  &  French  leur.  Of 
course  this  is  far  from  exhausting  the  list  of 
vowels  in  actual  use.— Tmnalator.l 


*  [That  is,  according  to  the  previous  direc- 
tions, to  keep  the  tongue  altogether  depressed, 
in  the  position  for  aw  in  gnaw,  which  is  not 
natural  for  an  Englishman,  so  that  for  English 
00  in  ^  we  may  expect  the  result  to  be  ma- 
terially different.— 7*ran«Zator.] 

t  [Prof.  Helmholtz  may  mean  the  Swedish 
o,  see  note  *  p.  io6d.  The  following  words  im- 
mediately preceding  the  notes,  which  oooar 
in  the  3rd  (German  edition,  appear  to  have 
been  accidentally  omitted  in  the  4th.  They 
are,  however,  retained  as  they  seem  neoessaiy. 
— Translator.} 

Digitized  by  V^OOQIC 


CHAP.  V.  7.  VOWEL  QUALITIES  OF  TONE.  iii 

or  too  open,  becoming  A".  But  if  the  b"\}  resonator  be  used,  which  is  an  Octave 
higher,  it  is  the  vowel  A  that  excites  the  strongest  sympathetic  resonance ;  while 
O,  which  was  so  powerful  with  the  b]}  resonator,  now  produces  only  a  slight  effect. 

For  the  high  upper  partials  of  A,  E,  I,  no  resonators  can  be  made  which  are 
capable  of  sensibly  reinforcing  them.  We  are,  then,  driven  principally  to  observa- 
tions made  with  the  unassisted  ear.  It  has  cost  me  much  trouble  to  determine  these 
strengthened  partial  tones  in  the  vowels,  and  I  was  not  acquainted  with  them  when 
my  previous  accounts  were  published.*  They  are  best  observed  in  high  notes  of 
women's  voices,  or  the  falsetto  of  men's  voices.  The  upper  partials  of  high  notes 
in  that  part  of  the  scale  are  not  so  nearly  of  the  same  pitch  as  those  of  deeper  notes, 
^  and  heneer  they  are  more  readily  distinguished.  On  b]),  for  example,  women's 
voices  could  easily  bring  out  all  the  vowels,  with  a  full  quality  of  tone,  but  at 
higher  pitches  the  choice  is  more  limited.  When  b^  is  sung,  then,  the  Twelfth/" 
is  heard  for  the  broad  A,  the  double  Octave  6'"b  for  E,  the  high  Third  ^i""  for  I,  f 
*all  clearly,  the  last  even  piercingly.     [See  table  on  p.  124,  note.]  f 

Further,  I  should  observe,  that  the  table  of  notes  given  on  the  preceding  page, 
relates  only  to  those  kinds  of  vowels  which  i^pear  to  me  to  have  the  most  cha- 
racteristic quality  of  tone,  but  that  in  addition  to  these,  all  intermediate  stages 
ure  possible,  passing  insensibly  from  one  to  the  other,  and  are  actually  used  partly 
in  dialects,  partly  by  particular  individuals,  partly  in  peculiar  pitches  while  singing, 
or  to  give  a  more  decided  character  while  whispering. 

It  is  easy  to  recognise,  and  indeed  is  sufficiently  well  known,  that  the  vowels 
with  a  single  resonance  from  U  through  0  to  clear  A  can  be  altered  in  continuous 
succession.  But  I  wish  further  to  remark,  since  doubts  have  been  thrown  on  the 
deep  resonance  I  have  assigned  to  U,  that  when  I  apply  to  my  ear  a  resonator 
tuned  to  /,  and,  singing  upon  f  or  B\}  b,9  the  fundamental  tone,  try  to  find  the 
vowel  resembling  U  which  has  the  strongest  resonance,  it  does  not  answer  to  a 
dull  U,  but  to  a  U  on  the  way  to  O.t  % 

Then  again  transitions  are  possible  between  the  vowels  of  the  A — 0 — U  series 
and  those  of  the  A — 0 — tT  series,  as  well  as  between  the  last  named  and  those  of 
the  A — E— I  series.  I  can  begin  on  the  position  for  U,  and  gradually  transform 
the  cavity  of  the  mouth,  already  narrowed,  into  the  tube-like  forms  for  0  and  Q, 
in  which  case  the  high  resonance  becomes  more  distinct  and  at  the  same  time 
higher,  the  narrower  the  tube  is  made.  If  we  make  this  transition  while  applying 
a  resonator  between  b]}  and  b']}  to  the  ear,  we  hear  the  loudness  of  the  tone 
increase  at  a  certain  stage  of  the  transition,  and  then  diminish  again.  The  higher 
the  resonator,  the  nearer  must  the  vowel  approach  to  0  or  tT.  With  a  proper 
position  of  the  mouth  the  reinforced  tone  may  be  brought  up  to  a  whistle.  Also 
in  a  gentle  whisper,  where  the  rustle  of  the  air  in  the  larynx  is  kept  very  weak,  so 
that  with  vowels  having  a  narrow  opening  of  the  mouth  it  can  be  scarcely  heard,  a 
strong  fricative  noise  in  the  opening  of  the  mouth  is  often  required  to  make  the 
vowel  audible.  That  is  to  say,  we  then  make  the  vowels  more  like  their  related  ^ 
consonants,  Enghsh  W  and  German  J  [English  Y]. 

Generally  speaking  the  vowels  §  with  double  resonance  admit  of  numerous 
modifications,  because  any  high  pitch  of  one  of  the  resonances  may  combine  with 
any  low  pitch  of  the  other.  This  is  best  studied  by  applying  a  resonator  to  the 
ear  and  trying  to  find  the  corresponding  vowel  degrees  in  the  three  series  which 
reinforce  its  tone,  and  then  endeavouring  to  pass  from  one  of  these  to  the  other  in 
such  a  way  that  the  resonator  should  have  a  reinforced  tone  throughout. 

♦   Gelekrte     Ameigen    der     Bayerischen  %  [An  U  Bound  verging  towards  O  is  gene- 

Akademieder  Wisseruchaften,  June  18,  1859.  rally  conceived  to  be  dfiiZ/cr,  not  brighter ^  by 

f  [The    passage   *  In    these  experiments  *  English  writers,  but  here  V  is  taken  as  the 

to '  too  deep  to  be  sensible,'  pp.  166-7  of  the  dullest  vowel.    This  remark  is  made  merely 

1st  English  edition,  is  here  cancelled,   and  to  prevent  confusion  with  English  readers. — 

p.  1 1 1&,  *  Further,  I  should  observe,'  to  p.  i  i6a,  Translator  J] 

'  high  tones  of  A,  E,  I,'  inserted  in  its  place  §  [Misprinted  Consonanten  in  the  German, 

from  the  4th  German  edition.— TratwZa/or.]  —Translator.']     ' 


Digitized  by 


google 


112  VOWEL  QUALITIES  OF  TONE.  pabt  i. 

Thus  the  resonator  V\}  answers  to  0,  to  an  Ao  and  to  an  E  which  resembles  A, 
and  these  sounds  may  pass  continuously  one  into  the  other. 

The  resonator  /  answers  to  the  transition  Ou — 0 — E.  The  resonator  d"  to 
Oa — ^Ad — A,.  In  a  similar  manner  each  of  the  higher  tones  may  be  connected 
with  various  deeper  tones.  Thus  assuming  a  position  of  the  mouth  which  would 
give  e^'^  for  whistling,  we  can,  without  changing*the  pitch  of  the  fricative  sound  in 
the  mouth,  whisper  a  vowel  inclining  to  0  or  inclining  to  tT,  by  allowing  the 
fricative  sound  in  the  larynx  to  have  a  higher  or  deeper  resonance  in  the  back  part 
of  the  mouth.* 

In  comparing  the  strength  of  the  upper  partials  of  different  vowels  by  means  of 
resonators,  it  is  further  to  be  remembered,  that  the  reinforcement  by  means  of  the 
resonance  of  the  mouth  affects  the  prime  tone  of  the  note  produced  by  the  voice, 
as  well  as  the  upper  partials.  And  as  it  is  especially  the  vibrations  of  the  prime, 
f  which  by  their  reaction  on  the  vocal  chords  retain  these  in  regular  vibratory  motion, 
the  voice  speaks  much  more  powerfully,  when  the  prime  itself  receives  such  a 
reinforcement.  This  is  especially  observable  in  those  parts  of  the  scale  which 
the  singer  reaches  with  difficulty.  It  may  also  be  noted  with  reed  pipes  having 
metal  tongues.  When  a  resonance  pipe  is  applied  to  them  tuned  to  the  tone  of  tl^e 
tongue,  or  a  little  higher,  extraordinarily  powerful  and  rich  tones  are  produced,  by 
means  of  strong  pressure  but  little  wind,  and  the  tongue  oscillates  in  large  ex- 
cursions either  way.  The  pitch  of  a  metal  tongue  becomes  a  little  flatter  than 
before.  This  is  not  perceived  with  the  human  voice  because  the  singer  is  able  to 
regulate  the  tension  of  the  vocal  chords  accordingly.  Thus  I  find  distinctly  that 
at  b]},  the  extremity  of  my  falsetto  voice,  I  can  sing  powerfully  an  0,  an  A,  and  an 
A  on  the  way  to  0,  which  have  their  resonance  at  this  pitch,  whereas  U,  if  it  is 
not  made  to  come  very  near  0,  and  I,  are  dull  and  uncertain,  with  the  expenditure 
of  more  air  than  in  the  former  case.  Begard  must  be  had  to  this  circumstance  in 
f  experiments  on  the  strength  of  upper  partials,  because  those  of  a  vowel  which  speaks 
powerfully,  may  become  proportionally  too  powerful,  as  compared  with  those  of  a 
vowel  which  speaks  weakly.  Thus  I  have  found  that  the  high  tones  of  the  soprano 
voice  which  lie  in  the  reinforcing  region  of  the  vowel  A  at  the  upper  extremity  of  the 
doubly-accented  [or  one-foot]  Octave,  when  sung  to  the  vowel  A,  exhibit  their 
higher  Octave  more  strongly  than  is  the  case  for  the  vowels  E  and  I,  which  do  not 
speak  so  well  although  the  latter  have  their  strong  resonance  at  the  upper  end  of 
the  thrice-accented  [or  six-inch]  Octave. 

It  has  been  already  remarked  (p.  39c)  that  the  strength  and  amplitude  of 
sympathetic  vibration  is  affected  by  the  mass  and  boundaries  of  the  body  which 
vibrates  sympathetically.  A  body  of  considerable  mass  which  can  perform  its 
vibrations  as  much  as  possible  without  any  hindrance  from  neighbouring  bodies, 
and  has  not  its  motion  damped  by  the  internal  friction  of  its  parts,  after  it  has 
once  been  excited,  can  continue  to  vibrate  for  a  long  time,  and  consequently,  if  it 
f  has  to  be  set  in  the  highest  degree  of  sympathetic  vibration,  the  oscillations  of  the 
exciting  tone  must,  for  a  comparatively  long  time,  coincide  with  those  proper 
vibrations  excited  in  itself.  That  is  to  say,  the  highest  degree  of  sympathetic 
resonance  can  be  produced  only  by  using  tones  which  he  within  very  narrow  limits 
of  pitch.  This  is  the  case  with  tuning-forks  and  bells.  The  mass  of  air  in  the 
cavity  of  the  mouth,  on  the  other  hand,  has  but  slight  density  and  mass,  its  walls, 
so  far  as  they  are  composed  of  soft  parts,  are  not  capable  of  offering  much  resist- 
ance, are  imperfectly  elastic,  and  when  put  in  vibration  have  much  internal  friction 
to  stop  their  motion.  Moreover  the  vibrating  mass  of  air  in  the  cavity  of  the 
mouth  conmiunicates  through  the  orifice  of  the  mouth  with  the  outer  air,  to  whicli 
it^rapidly  gives  off  large  parts  of  the  motion  it  has  received.    For  this  reason  a 

*  This  appears  to  me  to  meet  the  objec-  my  attention  to  the  habit  of  using  sach  devia. 

tions  whioh  were  made  by  Herr  G.  Engel,  in  tions  from  the  usual  qualities  of   vowels  in 

Beichart's  and  Du  Bois-Reymond's  Archiv.,  syllables  which  are  briefly  uttered. 
1869,  pp.  317-319*    Herr  J.  Stockhausen  drew 


Digitized  by 


Google 


CHAP.  V.  7.        MODIFICATIONS  OP  VOWEL  QUALITIES.  113 

vibratory  motion  once  excited  in  the  air  filling  the  cavity  of  the  mouth  is  very 
rapidly  extinguished,  as  any  one  may  easily  observe  by  filliping  his  cheek  with  a 
finger  when  the  mouth  is  put  into  different  vowel  positions.  We  thus  very  easily 
distinguish  the  pitch  of  the  resonance  for  the  various  transitional  degrees  from  0 
towards  U  in  one  direction  and  towards  A  in  the  other.  But  the  tone  dies  away 
rapidly.  The  various  resonances  of  the  cavity  of  the  mouth  can  also  be  made 
audible  by  rapping  the  teeth.  Just  for  this  reason  a  tone,  which  oscillates  approxi- 
mately in  agreement  with  the  few  vibrations  of  such  a  brief  resonance  tone,  will  be 
reinforced  by  sympathetic  vibration  to  an  extent  not  much  less  than  another  tone 
which  exactly  coincides  with  the  first ;  and  the  raoge  of  tones  which  can  thus 
be  sensibly  reinforced  by  a  given  position  of  the  mouth,  is  rather  considerable.* 
This  is  confirmed  by  experiment.  When  I  apply  a  b]}  resonator  to  the  right, 
and  an  /'  resonator  to  the  left  ear  and  sing  the  vowel  0  on  ^,  I  find  a  reinforce- 
ment not  only  of  the  4th  partial  b]}  which  answers  to  the  proper  tone  of  the  ^ 
cavity  of  the  mouth,  but  also,  very  perceptibly,  though  considerably  less,  of  /', 
the  6th  partial,  also.  If  I  then  change  0  into  an  A,  until  /'  finds  its  strongest 
resonance,  the  reinforcement  of  b^}  does  not  entirely  disappear  although  it  becomes 
much  less. 

The  position  of  the  mouth  from  0  to  0^  appears  to  be  that  which  is  most 
favourable  for  the  length  of  its  proper  tone  and  the  production  of  a  resonance 
limited  to  a  very  narrow  range  of  pitch.  At  least,  as  I  have  before  remarked,  for 
this  position  the  reinforcement  of  a  suitable  tuning-fork  is  most  powerful,  and  tap- 
ping the  cheek  or  the  lips  gives  the  most  distinct  tone.  If  then  for  0  the  rein- 
forcement by  resonance  extends  to  the  interval  of  a  Fifth,  the  intervals  will  be  stiU 
greater  for  the  other  vowels.  With  this  agree  experiments.  Apply  any  resonator 
to  the  ear,  take  a  suitable  under  tone,  sing  the  different  vowels  to  this  under  tone,  and 
let  one  vowel  melt  into  another.  The  greatest  reinforcements  by  resonance  take 
place  on  that  vowel  or  those  vowels,  for  which  one  of  the  characteristic  tones  in  f 
the  diagram  p.  1006  coincides  with  the  proper  tone  of  the  resonator.  But  more  or 
less  considerable  reinforcement  is  also  observed  for  such  vowels  as  have  their  charac- 
teristic tones  at  moderate  differences  of  pitch  from  the  proper  tone  of  the  resonator, 
and  the  reinforcement  will  be. less  the  greater  these  differences  of  pitch. 

By  this  means  it  becomes  possible  in  general  to  distinguish  the  vowels  from 
each  other  even  when  the  note  to  which  they  are  sung  is  not  precisely  one  of  the 
harmonic  under  tones  of  the  vowels.  From  the  second  partial  tone  onwards,  the 
intervals  are  narrow  enough  for  one  or  two  of  the  partials  to  be  distinctly  reinforced 
by  the  resonance  of  the  mouth.  It  is  only  when  the  proper  tone  of  the  cavity  of 
the  mouth  falls  midway  between  the  prime  tone  of  the  note  sung  by  the  voice  and 
its  higher  Octave,  or  is  more  than  a  Fifth  deeper  than  that  prime  tone,  that  the 
characteristic  resonance  will  be  weak. 

Now  in  speaking,  both  sexes  choose  one  of  the  deepest  positions  of  their  voice. 
Men  generally  choose  the  upper  half  of  the  great  (or  eight-foot)  Octave ;  and  If 
women  the  upper  half  of  the  smaU  (or  four-foot)  Octave.f  With  the  exception  of 
U,  which  admits  of  fluctuations  in  its  proper  tone  of  nearly  an  Octave,  all  these 
pitches  of  the  speaking  voice  have  the  corresponding  proper  tones  of  the  cavity 
of  the  mouth  situated  within  sufficiently  narrow  intervals  from  the  upper  partials  of 
the  speaking  tone  to  create  sensible  resonance  of  one  or  more  of  these  partials, 
and  thus  characterise  the  vowel.t  To  this  must  be  added  that  the  speaking  voice, 
probably  through  great  pressure  of  the  vocal  hgaments  upon  one  another,  converting 

*  On  this  subject  see  Appendix  X.,  and  of  certain  of  its  partials  with  exact  pitches 

the  corresponding  investigation  in  the  text  in  but  in  their  coming  near  enough  to  thc^ 

Part  I.  Chap.  VI.  therein  referred  to.  pitches  to  receive  reinforcement,  and  that  the 

t  [That  is  both  use  their  *  lower  thick '  character  of  a  vowel  quality  of  tone,  like  that 

register,  as  described  in  the  note  p.  loid^  but  of  all  qualities  of  tone,  depends  not  on  the 

are  an  Octave  apart.—  Translator.]  absolute  pitch,  but  on  the  relative  force  of  the 

t  [Observe  here  that  the  quality  of  the  upper  partials.    As  Prof.  Helmholtz's  theory 

vowel  tone  is  not  made  to  consist  in  the  identity  has  often  been  grievously  misunderstood,  I 


Digitized  by  VjOt)Ql€ 


114  MODIFICATIONS  OF  VOWEL  QUALITIES.  parti. 

them  into  striking  reedi,  has  a  jarring  quality  of  tone,  that  is,  possesses  stronger 
upper  partials  than  the  singing  voice. 

In  singing,  on  the  other  hand,  especially  at  higher  pitches,  conditions  are  less 
favourable  for  the  characterisation  of  vowels.  Every  one  knows  that  it  is  generally 
much  more  difficult  to  understand  words  when  sung  than  when  spoken,  and  that 
the  difficulty  is  less  with  male  than  with  female  voices,  each  having  been  equally  well 
cultivated.  Were  it  otherwise,  '  books  of  the  words '  at  operas  and  concerts  would 
be  unnecessary.  Above  /,  the  characterisation  of  U  becomes  imperfect  even  if  it 
is  closely  assimilated  to  0.  But  so  long  as  it  remains  the  only  vowel  of  indetermi- 
nate sound,  and  the  remainder  allow  of  sensible  reinforcement  of  their  upper  partials 
in  certain  regions,  this  negative  character  wiU  distinguish  U.  On  the  other  hand 
a  soprano  voice  in  the  neighbourhood  of/'  should  not  be  able  to  clearly  distinguish 
U,  0,  and  A ;  and  this  agrees  with  my  own  experience.    On  singing  the  three  vowels 

^  in  ixomediate  succession,  the  resonance  /''  for  A  will,  however,  be  still  somewhat 
clearer  in  the  cavity  of  the  mouth  when  tuned  for  b"\}j  than  when  it  is  tuned  to  b^^ 
for  0.  The  soprano  voice  will  in  this  case  be  able  to  make  the  A  clearer,  by  eleva- 
ting the  pitch  of  the  cavity  of  the  mouth  towards  t2'''  and  thus  making  it  approach 
to/''.  The  0,  on  the  other  hand,  can  be  separated  from  U  by  approaching  0«,  and 
giving  the  prime  more  decisive  force.  Nevertheless  these  vowels,  if  not  sung  in 
immediate  succession,  will  not  be  very  clearly  distinguished  by  a  listener  who  is 
unacquainted  with  the  mode  of  pronouncing  the  vowels  that  the  soprano  singer 
uses.* 

A  further  means  of  helping  to  discriminate  vowels,  moreover,  is  found  in  com- 
mencing them  powerfully.  This  depends  upon  a  general  relation  in  bodies  excited 
to  sympathetic  vibration.  Thus,  if  we  excite  sympathetic  vibration  in  a  suitable 
body  with  a  tone  somewhat  different  from  its  proper  tone,  by  commencing  it  suddenly 
with  great  power,  we  hear  at  first,  in  addition  to  the  exciting  tone  which  is  rein- 

^  forced  by  resonance,  the  proper  tone  of  the  sympathetically  vibrating  body.f  Bat 
the  latter  soon  dies  away,  while  the  first  remains.  In  the  case  of  tuning-forks  with 
laige  resonator,  we  can  even  hear  beats  between  the  two  tones.  Apply  a  b');}  resonator 
to  the  ear,  and  commence  singing  the  vowel  0  powerfully  on  g,  of  which  the  upper 
partials  g^  and  d"  have  only  a  weak  lasting  resonance  in  the  cavity  of  the  mouth* 
and  you  may  hear  immediately  at  the  commencement  of  the  vowel,  a  short  sharp 
beat  between  the  b^  of  the  cavity  of  the  mouth  and  of  the  resonator.  On  selecting 
another  vowel,  this  51>  vanishes,  which  shews  that  the  pitch  of  the  cavity  of  the 
mouth  helps  to  generate  it.  In  this  case  then  also  the  sudden  commencement  of 
the  tones  g^  and  df^  belonging  to  the  compound  tone  of  the  voice,  excites  the  inter- 
mediate proper  tone  &t>  of  the  cavity  of  the  mouth,  which  rapidly  hdea.  The 
same  thing  may  be  observed  for  other  pitches  of  the  resonator  used,  when  we  sing 
notes,  powerfully  commenced,  which  have  upper  partials  that  are  not  reinforced  by 
the  resonator,  provided  that  a  vowel  is  chosen  with  a  characteristic  pitch  which 
answers  to  the  pitch  of  the  resonator.  Hence  it  results  that  when  any  vowel  in 
any  pitch  is  powerfully  commenced,  its  characteristic  tone  becomes  audible  as  a 
1  short  beat.  By  this  means  the  vowel  may  be  distinctly  characterised  at  the 
jmoment  of  commencement,  even  when  it  becomes  intermediate  on  long  con- 
.  tinuance.  But  for  this  purpose,  as  already  remarked,  an  exact  and  energetic  com- 
mencement is  necessary.  How  much  such  a  commencement  assists  in  rendering 
the  words  of  a  singer  intelligible  is  well  known.  For  this  reason  also  the  vocal- 
isation of  the  briefly  uttered  words  of  a  reciting  parlando,  is  more  distinct  than 
that  of  sustained  song4 

draw  particular  attention  to  the  point  in  this  may  make  in  the  vowels  in  English,  German, 

place.    See  also  the  table  which  I  have  added  French  and  Italian,  at  different  pitches,  so  as 

in  a  footnote  on  p.  i24d,— Translator.']  to  remain  intelligible.— TrntuZotor.] 

♦  [In  my  Pronunciation  for  Singers  (Cur-  f  See  the  mathematical  statement  of  this  pro- 
wen,  1877),  and  my  Speech  in  Song  (Novello,  cess  in  App.  IX.,  remarks  on  equations  4  to  46. 
1S78)  I  have  endeavoured  to  give  a  popular  I  The  facts  here  adduced  meet,  I  think,  the 
explanation  cf  the  alterations  which  a  singer  objections  brought  against  my  vowel  theory  by 

Digitized  by  V^OOQIC 


CHAP.  V.  7.  CHARACTERISTICS   OF  VOWELS.  115 

Moreover  vowels  admit  of  other  kinds  of  alterations  in  their  qnalities  of  tone, 
conditioned  by  alterations  of  their  characteristic  tones  within  certain  limits.  Thus 
the  resonating  capability  of  the  cavity  of  the  month  may  undergo  in  general  altera- 
tions in  strength  and  definition,  which  would  render  the  character  of  the  various 
vowels  and  their  difference  from  one  another  in  general  more  or  less  conspicuous 
or  obscure.  Flaccid  sofi;  walls  in  any  passage  with  sonorous  masses  of  air,  are 
generally  prejudicial  to  the  force  of  the  vibrations.  Partly  too  much  of  the  motion 
is  given  off  to  the  outside  through  the  soft  masses,  partly  too  much  is  destroyed  by 
friction  within  them.  Wooden  organ  pipes  have  a  less  energetic  quality  of  tone 
than  metal  ones,  and  those  of  pasteboard  a  still  duller  quality,  even  when  the 
mouthpiece  remains  unaltered.  The  walls  of  the  human  throat,  and  the  cheeks, 
are,  however,  much  more  yielding  than  pasteboard.  Hence  if  the  tone  of  the  voice 
with  all  its  partials  is  to  meet  with  a  powerful  resonance  and  come  out  unweakened, 
these  most  flaccid  parts  of  the  passage  for  our  voice,  must  be  as  much  as  possible  f 
thrown  out  of  action,  or  else  rendered  elastic  by  tension,  and  in  addition  the  passage 
must  be  made  as  short  and  wide  as  possible.  The  last  is  effected  by  raising  the 
larynx.  The  sofli  wall  of  the  cheeks  can  be  almost  entirely  avoided,  by  taking  care 
that  the  rows  of  teeth  are  not  too  far  apart.  The  lips,  when  their  co-operation  is 
not  necessary,  as  it  is  for  0  and  tT,  may  be  held  so  far  apart  that  the  sharp  firm 
edges  of  the  teeth  define  the  orifice  of  the  mouth.  For  A  the  angles  of  the  mouth 
can  be  drawn  entirely  aside.  For  0  they  can  be  firmly  stretched  by  the  muscles 
above  and  below  them  (levator  anguli  oris  and  triangukms  menti),  which  then  feel 
like  stretched  cords  to  the  touch,  and  can  be  thus  pressed  against  the  teeth,  so  that 
this  part  of  the  margin  of  the  orifice  of  the  mouth  is  also  made  sharp  and  capable 
of  resisting. 

In  the  attempt  to  produce  a  clear  energetic  tone  of  the  voice  we  also  become 
aware  of  the  tension  of  a  large  number  of  muscles  lying  in  front  of  the  throat, 
both  those  which  he  between  the  under  jaw  and  the  tongue-bone  and  help  to  form  ^ 
the  floor  of  the  cavity  of  the  mouth  {mylohyoideu8y  geniohyoideus,  and  perhaps 
also  hiventer),  and  likewise  those  which  run  down  near  the  larynx  and  air  tubes,  and 
draw  down  the  tongue-bone  (st&mokyoidetis,  stemothyroideus  and  thyrohyoidetts). 
Without  the  coxmteraction  of  the  latter,  indeed,  considerable  tension  of  the  former 
would  be  impossible.  Besides  this  a  contraction  of  the  skin  on  both  sides  of  the 
larynx  which  takes  place  at  the  commencement  of  the  tone  of  the  voice,  shews  that 
the  omohyoideus  muscle,  which  runs  obliquely  down  from  the  tongue-bone  back- 
wards to  the  shoulder-blade,  is  also  stretched.  Without  its  co-operation  the  muscles 
arising  from  the  under  jaw  and  breast-bone  would  draw  the  larynx  too  far  forwards. 
Now  the  greater  part  of  these  muscles  do  not  go  to  the  larynx  at  all,  but  only  to 
the  tongue-bone,  from  which  the  larynx  is  suspended.  Hence  they  cannot  directly 
assist  in  the  formation  of  the  voice,  so  far  as  this  depends  upon  the  action  of  the 
larynx.  The  action  of  these  muscles,  so  far  as  I  have  been  able  to  observe  it  on 
myself,  is  also  much  less  when  I  utter  a  dull  guttural  A,  than  when  I  endeavour  to  ^ 
change  it  into  a  ringing,  keen  and  powerfully  penetrating  A.  Ringing  and  keen, 
applied  to  a  quality  of  tone,  imply  many  and  powerful  upper  partials,  and  the 
stronger  they  are,  of  course  the  more  marked  are  the  differences  of  the  vowels 
which  their  own  differences  condition.  A  singer,  or  a  declaimer,  will  occasionally 
interpose  among  his  bright  and  rich  tones  others  of  a  duller  character  as  a  contrast. 
Sharp  characterisation  of  vowel  quality  is  suitable  for  energetic,  joyful  or  vigorous 
frames  of  mind ;  indifferent  and  obscure  quality  of  tone  for  sad  and  troubled,  or  taci- 
turn states.  In  the  latter  case  speakers  like  to  change  the  proper  tone  of  the  vowels, 
by  drawing  the  extremes  closer  to  a  middle  Ad  (say  the  short  German  E  [the  final 

HeiT  E.  y.  Qnanten  (Poggendorff*s  Annal.,  article,  pp.  724-741,  with  espeolal  reference  to 
▼oL  cUt.  pp.  272  and  522),  so  far  as  they  do  not  it.  In  oonseqaence  of  the  new  matter  added 
rest  upon  misconceptions.  [In  the  ist  edition  by  Prof.  HehxLholtz  in  his  4th  German  edition 
of  this  translation,  during  the  printing  of  which  here  followed,  this  article  is  omitted  from  the 
V.  Quanten*8  first  paper  appeared,  I  added  an      present  edition. — Translator.]  j 

Digitized  by  V^OQ^lC 


ii6  VOWEL  QUALITIES  OF  TONE.  parti. 

English  obscare  A  in  idea]),  and  heiice  select  somewhat  deeper  tones  in  place  of  the 
high  tones  of  A,  E,  I. 

A  pecuhar  circumstance  must  also  be  mentioned  which  distinguishes  the 
human  voice  from  all  other  instruments  and  has  a  peculiar  relation  to  the  human 
ear.  Above  the  higher  reinforced  partial  tones  of  I,  in  the  neighbourhood  of  e"'' 
up  to  g'^"  [2640  to  3168  vib.]  the  notes  of  a  pianoforte  have  a  peculiar  cutting 
effect,  and  we  might  be  easily  led  to  believe  that  the  hammers  were  too  hard,  or 
that  their  mechanism  somewhat  differed  from  that  of  the  adjacent  notes.  But  the 
phenomenon  is  the  same  on  all  pianofortes,  and  if  a  very  small  glass  tube  or  sphere 
is  applied  to  the  ear,  the  cutting  effect  ceases,  and  these  notes  become  as  soft  and  weak 
as  the  rest,  but  another  and  deeper  series  of  notes  now  becomes  stronger  and  more 
cutting.  Hence  it  foUows  that  the  human  ear  by  its  own  resonance  favours  the  tones 
between  c""  and  ^"",  or,  in  other  words,  that  it  is  timed  to  one  of  these  pitches.* 

^  These  notes  produce  a  feeling  of  pain  in  sensitive  ears.  Hence  the  upper  partial 
tones  which  have  nearly  this  pitch,  if  any  such  exist,  are  extremely  prominent 
and  affect  the  ear  powerfully.  This  is  generally  the  case  for  the  human  voice  when 
it  is  strained,  and  will  help  to  give  it  a  screaming  effect.  In  powerful  male  voices 
singing  forte,  these  partial  tones  sound  Uke  a  clear  tinkling  of  little  bells,  accom- 
panying the  voice,  and  are  most  audible  in  choruses,  when  the  singers  shout  a 
little.  Every  individual  male  voice  at  such  pitches  produces  dissonant  upper  partials. 
When  basses  sing  their  high  e\  the  7th  partial  tonef  is  d"",  the  8th  e"",  the 
9th/'"#,  and  the  loth  g'"%  Now,  if  e""  and/'"#  are  loud,  and  d""  and  sr""Jf, 
though  weaker,  are  audible,  there  is  of  course  a  sharp  dissonance.  If  many  voices 
are  sounding  together,  producing  these  upper  partials  with  small  differences  of 
pitch,  the  result  is  a  very  peculiar  kind  of  tinkling,  which  is  readily  recognised  a 
second  time  when  attention  has  been  once  drawn  to  it.  I  have  not  noticed  any 
difference  of  effect  for  different  vowels  in  this  case,  but  the  tinkling  ceases  as  soon 

^  as  the  voices  are  taken  piano;  although  the  tone  produced  by  a  chorus  will  of 
course  still  have  considerable  power.  This  kind  of  tinkling  is  peculiar  to  htmian 
voices  ;  orchestral  instruments  do  not  produce  it  in  the  same  way  either  so  sensibly 
or  so  powerfully.  I  have  never  heard  it  from  any  other  musical  instrument  so 
clearly  as  from  human  voices. 

The  same  upper  partials  are  heard  also  in  soprano  voices  when  they  sing  forte  ; 
in  harsh,  uncertain  voices  they  are  tremulous,  and  hence  shew  some  resemblance 
to  the  tinkling  heard  in  the  notes  of  male  voices.  But  I  have  heard  them  brought 
out  with  exact  purity,  and  continue  to  sound  on  perfectly  and  quietly,  in  some 
steady  and  harmonious  female  voices,  and  also  in  some  excellent  tenor  voices.  In 
the  melodic  progression  of  a  voice  part,  I  then  hear  these  high  upper  partials  of 
the  four-times  accented  Octave,  falling  and  rising  at  different  times  within  the 
compass  of  a  minor  Third,  according  as  different  upper  partials  of  the  notes  snng 
enter  the  region  for  which  our  ear  is  so  sensitive.    It  is  certainly  remarkable  that 

^  it  should  be  precisely  the  human  voice  which  is  so  rich  in  those  upper  partials  for 
which  the  human  ear  is  so  sensitive.  Madame  E.  Seiler,  however,  remarks  that 
dogs  are  also  very  sensitive  for  the  high  e""  of  the  violin. 

This  reinforcement  of  the  upper  partial  tones  belonging  to  the  middle  of  the 
four-times  accented  Octave,  has,  however,  nothing  to  do  with  the  characterisation 
of  vowels.  I  have  mentioned  it  here,  merely  because  these  high  tones  are  readily 
remarked  in  investigations  into  the  vowel  qualities  of  tone,  and  the  observer  must 
not  be  misled  to  consider  them  as  peculiar  characteristics  of  individual  vowels. 
They  are  simply  a  characteristic  of  strained  voices. 

The  humming  tone  heard  when  singing  with  closed  mouth,  Ues  nearest  to  U. 

*  I  have  lately  found  that  my  right  ear  is  applying  a  short  paper  tuhe  to  the  entrance  of 

most  sensitive  for  /"",  and  my  left  for  c"".  my  ear,  this  chirp  is  rendered  extraordinarily 

When  I  drive  air  into  the  passage  leading  to  the  weak. 

tympanmn,  the  resonance  descends  to  c""Z  and  f  [The  first  six  partial  tones  are  «',  c",  6", 

g'"U  .    The  chirp  of  the  cricket  corresponds  pre-  e"',  g'^'U  ,  &"',  the  seventh  is  27  cents  flatter  than 

ciaely  to  the  higher  resonance,  and  on  merely  d'"*,— Translator.] 


Digitized  by  VjOOQIC 


CHAP.  v.  7. 


VOWEL  QUALITIES  OP  TONE. 


117 


This  hum  is  ased  in  uttering  the  conson^its  M,  N  and  N^.  The  size  of  the  exit 
of  the  air  (the  nostrils)  is  in  this  case  much  smaller  in  comparison  with  the 
resonant  chamber  (the  internal  nasal  cavity)  than  the  opening  of  the  lips  for  U  in 
comparison  with  the  corresponding  resonant  chamber  in  the  mouth.  Hence,  in 
humming,  the  peculiarities  of  the  U  tone  are  much  enhanced.  Thus  although 
upper  partials  are  present,  even  up  to  a  considerably  high  pitch,  yet  they  decrease 
in  strength  as  they  rise  in  pitch  much  faster  than  for  U.  The  upper  Octave  is 
tolerably  strong  in  humming,  but  all  the  higher  partial  tones  are  weak.  Humming 
in  the  N-position  differs  a  little  horn  that  in  the  M-position,  by  having  its  upper 
partials  less  damped  than  for  M.  But  it  is  only  at  the  instant  when  the  cavity  of 
the  mouth  is  opened  or  closed  that  a  clear  difference  exists  between  these  conso- 
nants. We  cannot  enter  into  the  details  of  the  composition  of  the  sound  of  the 
other  consonants,  because  they  produce  noises  which  have  no  constant  pitch,  and 
are  not  musical  tones,  to  which  we  have  here  to  confine  our  attention.  f 

The  theory  of  vowel  sounds  here  explained  may  be  confirmed  by  experiments 
with  artificial  reed  pipes,  to  which  proper  resonant  chambers  are  attached.  This 
was  first  done  by  Willis,  who  attached  reed  pipes  to  cylindrical  chambers  of  variable 
length,  and  produced  different  tones  by  increasing  the  length  of  the  resonant  tube. 
The  shortest  tubes  gave  him  I,  and  then  E,  A,  0,  up  to  U,  until  the  tube  exceeded 
the  length  of  a  quarter  of  a  wave.  On  further  increasing  the  length  the  vowels 
returned  in  converse  order.  His  determination  of  the  pitch  of  the  resonant  pipes 
agrees  well  with  mine  for  the  deeper  vowels.  The  pitch  foxmd  by  WilUs  for  the 
higher  vowels  was  relatively  too  high,  because  in  this  case  the  length  of  the  wave 
was  smaller  than  the  diameter  of  the  tubes,  and  consequently  the  usual  calcula- 
tion of  pitch  firom  the  length  of  the  tubes  alone  was  no  longer  applicable.  The 
vowels  E  and  I  were  also  far  from  accurately  resembling  those  of  the  voice,  because 
the  second  resonance  was  absent,  and  hence,  as  WiUis  himself  states,  they  could 
not  be  well  distinguished.*  f 


Vowel 

In  the  Word 

Pitch, 
Willis 

Pitch, 
Helmholtx 

Length  of  Tube 
in  Inches 

0 

No 

c" 

c" 

47 

A» 

Nought 

e"b 

e"b 

3-8 

Paw 

9" 

f 

305 

A 

Part        ^ 

d'"b 

d"'b 

2*2 

Pad 

r 

1-8 

£ 

Pay          ^ 

d"" 

h"'b 

i-o 

Pet 

*■           fjntf 

d'" 

0-6 

I 

See 

r' 

d"" 

038  (?) 

The  vowels  are  obtained  much  more  clearly  and  distinctly  with  properly  tuned 
resonators,  than  with  cylindrical  resonance  chambers.  On  applying  to  a  reed  pipe 
which  gave  i|>,  a  glass  resonator  tuned  to  l^,  I  obtained  the  vowel  U  ;  changing  H 
the  resonator  to  one  tuned  for  h*);},  I  obtained  0  ;  the  h"\}  resonator  gave  a  rather 
close  A,  and  the  d'"  resonator  a  clear  A.  Hence  by  tuning  the  applied  chambers 
in  the  same  way  we  obtain  the  same  vowels  quite  independently  of  the  form  of  the 
chamber  and  nature  of  its  walls.    I  also  succeeded  in  producing  various  grades  of 


*  [Probably  the  first  treatise  on  phonology 
in  which  Willises  experiments  were  given  at 
length,  and  the  above  table  cited,  with  Wheat- 
stone's  article  from  the  London  and  Westmin- 
ster Bemew^  which  was  kindly  brought  under 
my  notice  by  Sir  Charles  Wheatstone  himself, 
was  my  Alphabet  of  Nature j  London ,  1 845.  The 
table  includes  U  exemplified  by  but^  boot^  with 
an  indefinite  length  of  pipe.  The  word  pad  is 
misprinted  paa  in  all  the  Oerman  editions  of 
Helmholtz  (even  the  4th,  which  appeared  after 
the  correction  in  my  translation),  and  as  he 


therefore  could  not  separate  its  A  from  that  in 
part,  he  gives  no  pitch.  It  is  really  the  nearest 
English  representative  of  the  German  .  The 
sounds  in  noughtt  paw,  which  Sir  John  Her- 
schel,  when  citing  Willis  (Art.  *  Sound,'  in 
Erusyc.  Metropol.,  par.  375),  could  not  distin- 
guish, were  probably  meant  for  the  broad 
Italian  open  O,  or  English  o  in  more,  and  the 
English  aw  in  maw  respectively.  The  length 
of  the  pipe  in  inches  is  here  added  from  Willis's 
paper.  I  have  heard  Willis's  pxperiments 
repeated  by  Wheatstone.  —  jTraiw/ator.l 


Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC' 


ii8  VOWEL  QUALITIES  OF  TONE.  part  !• 

A,  0,  E,  and  I  with  the  same  reed  pipe,  by  applying  glass  spheres  into  whose  external 
opening  glass  tubes  were  inserted  from  6  to  lo  centimetres  (2*36  to  3*94  inches)  in 
length,  in  order  to  imitate  the  doable  resonance  of  the  oral  cavity  for  these 
vowels. 

Willis  has  also  given  another  interesting  method  for  producing  vowels.  If  a 
toothed  wheel,  with  many  teeth,  revolve  rapidly,  and  a  spring  be  applied  to  its 
teeth,  the  spring  wiU  be  raised  by  each  tooth  as  it  passes,  and  a  tone  will  be  pro- 
duced having  its  pitch  number  equal  to  the  number  of  teeth  by  which  it  has  been 
struck  in  a  second.  Now  if  one  end  of  the  spring  is  well  £a>stened,  and  the  spring 
be  set  in  vibration,  it  will  itself  produce  a  tone  which  will  increase  in  pitch  as  the 
spring  diminishes  in  length.  If  then  we  turn  the  wheel  with  a  constant  velocity, 
and  allow  a  watch  spring  of  variable  length  to  strike  against  its  teeth,  we  shall 
obtain  for  a  long  spring  a  quality  of  tone  resembling  U,  and  as  we  shorten  the 

f  spring  other  qualities  in  succession  like  0,  A,  E,  I,  the  tone  of  the  spring  here 
playing  the  part  of  the  reinforced  tone  which  determines  the  vowel.  But  this 
imitation  of  the  vowels  is  certainly  much  less  complete  than  that  obtaaned  by  reed 
pipes.  The  reason  of  this  process  also  evidently  depends  upon  our  produciag 
compound  tones  in  which  certain  upper  partials  (which  in  this  case  correspond  with 
the  proper  tones  of  the  spring  itse^)  are  more  reinforced  than  others. 

WiUis  himself  advanced  a  theory  concerning  the  nature  of  vowel  tones  which 
differs  from  that  I  have  laid  down  in  agreement  with  the  whole  connection  of  all 
other  acoustical  phenomena.  Willis  imagines  that  the  pulses  of  air  which  produce 
the  vowel  qualities,  are  themselves  tones  which  rapidly  die  away,  corresponding  to 
the  proper  tone  of  the  spring  in  his  last  experiment,  or  the  short  echo  produced  by 
a  pulse  or  a  little  explosion  of  air  in  the  mouth,  or  in  the  resonance  chamber  of  a 
reed  pipe.  In  fact  something  like  the  sound  of  a  vowel  will  be  heard  if  we  only 
tap  against  the  teeth  with  a  little  rod,  and  set  the  cavity  of  the  mouth  in  the  posi- 

f  tion  required  for  the  different  vowels.  Willis's  description  of  the  motion  of  sound 
for  vowels  is  certainly  not  a  great  way  from  the  truth ;  but  it  only  assigns  the 
mode  in  which  the  motion  of  the  air  ensues,  and  not  the  corresponding  reaction 
which  this  produces  in  the  ear.  That  this  kind  of  motion  as  well  as  all  others 
is  actually  resolved  by  the  ear  into  a  series  of  partial  tones,  according  to  the  laws 
of  sympathetic  resonance,  is  shewn  by  the  agreement  of  the  analysis  of  vowel 
qualities  of  tone  made  by  the  unarmed  ear  and  by  the  resonators.  This  will 
appear  still  more  clearly  in  the  next  chapter,  where  experiments  will  be  described 
shewing  the  direct  composition  of  vowel  qualities  from  their  partial  tones. 

Vowel  qualities  of  tone  consequently  are  essentially  distinguished  from  the 
tones  of  most  other  musical  instruments  by  the  fact  that  the  loudness  of  their 
partial  tones  does  not  depend  solely  upon  their  numerical  order  but  preponder- 
antly upon  the  absolute  pitch  of  those  partials.  Thus  when  I  sing  the  vowel  A  to 
the  note  ^,*  the  reinforced  tone  b"\}  is  the  12th  partial  of  the  compound  tone  ; 

%  and  when  I  sing  the  same  vowel  A  to  the  note  6^,  the  reinforced  tone  is  still  &'^, 
but  is  now  the  2nd  partial  of  the  compound  tone  sung.f 


From  the  examples  adduced  to  shew  the  dependence  of  quality  of  tone  from 
the  mode  in  which  a  musical  tone  is  compounded,  we  may  deduce  the  following 
general  rules : — 

1.  Simple  Tones f  like  those  of  tuning-forks  applied  to  resonance  chambers  and 
wide  stopped  organ  pipes,  have  a  very  soft,  pleasant  sound,  free  from  aU  roughness, 
but  wanting  in  power,  and  dull  at  low  pitches. 

2.  Musical  Tones,  which  are  accompanied  by  a  moderately  loud  series  of  the 

*  [Eb  has  for  2nd  partial  «b,  for  3rd  6b,  t  [See  App.  XX.  sec.  M.  No.  i,  for   Jen- 

and  hence  for  6th  b'b,  and  for   12th,  b"b. —      kin  and  Ewing^s  analysis  of  vowel  sounds  by 
TransUUor.]  means  of  the  Phonograph,— rransfattor.] 

Digitized  by  VjOOQlC 


CHAPB.v.vi.     APPREHENSION  OF  QUALITIES  OF  TONE.  119 

lower  partial  tones,  up  to  about  the  sixth  partial,  are  more  harmonious  and 
musical.  Compared  with  simple  tones  they  are  rich  and  splendid,  while  they  are 
at  the  same  time  perfectly  sweet  and  soft  if  the  higher  upper  partials  are  absent. 
To  these  belong  the  musical  tones  produced  by  the  pianoforte,  open  organ  pipes, 
the  softer  piano  tones  of  the  human  voice  and  of  the  French  horn.  The  last- 
namad  tones  form  the  transition  to  musical  tones  with  high  upper  partials  ;  while 
the  tones  of  flutes,  and  of  pipes  on  the  flue-stops  of  organs  with  a  low  pressure 
of  wind,  approach  to  simple  tones. 

3.  If  only  the  unevenly  numbered  partials  are  present  (as  in  narrow  stopped 
organ  pipes,  pianoforte  strings  struck  in  their  middle  points,  and  clarinets),  the 
quality  of  tone  is  hollow,  and,  when  a  large  number  of  such  upper  partials  are 
present,  ncuai.  When  the  prime  tone  predominates  the  quality  of  tone  is  rich ; 
but  when  the  prime  tone  is  not  sufficiently  superior  in  strength  to  the  upper 
partialfl,  the  quality  of  tone  is  poor.  Thus  the  quality  of  tone  in  the  wider  open  ^ 
organ  pipes  is  richer  than  that  in  the  narrower ;  i^trings  struck  with  pianoforte 
hammers  give  tones  of  a  richer  quality  than  when  struck  by  a  stick  or  plucked 
by  the  finger ;  the  tones  of  reed  pipes  with  suitable  resonance  chambers  have  a 
richer  quality  than  those  without  resonance  chambers. 

4*  When  partial  tones  higher  than  the  sixth  or  seventh  are  very  distinct,  the 
quality  of  tone  is  cutting  and  rough.  The  reason  for  this  will  be  seen  hereafter  to 
lie  in  the  dissonances  which  they  form  with  one  another.  The  degree  of  harshness 
may  be  very  different.  When  their  force  is  inconsiderable  the  higher  upper  partials 
do  not  essentially  detract  from  the  musical  applicability  of  the  compound  tones ; 
on  the  contrary,  they  are  useful  in  giving  character  and  expression  to  the  music. 
The  most  important  musical  tones  of  this  description  are  those  of  bowed  instru- 
ments and  of  most  reed  pipes,  oboe  (hautbois),  bassoon  (febgotto),  harmonium,  and 
the  human  voice.  The  rough,  braying  tones  of  brass  instruments  are  extremely 
penetrating,  and  hence  are  better  adapted  to  give  the  impression  of  great  power  ^ 
than  similar  tones  of  a  softer  quality.  They  are  consequently  little  suitable  for 
artistic  music  when  used  alone,  but  produce  great  effect  in  an  orchestra.  Why 
high  dissonant  upper  partials  should  make  a  musical  tone  more  penetrating  will 
appear  hereafter. 


CHAPTER  VI. 

ON  THE  APPBEHENSION  OF  QUALITIES  OF  TONE. 

Up  to  this  point  we  have  not  endeavoured  to  analyse  given  musical  tones  further 
than  to  determine  the  differences  in  the  number  and  loudness  of  their  partial  tones. 
Before  we  can  determine  the  function  of  the  ear  in  apprehending  qualities  of  tone,  T 
we  must  inquire  whether  a  determinate  relative  strength  of  the  upper  partials 
suffices  to  give  us  the  impression  of  a  determinate  musical  quality  of  tone  or 
whether  there  are  not  also  other  perceptible  differences  in  qualil^  which  are 
independent  of  such  a  relation.  Since  we  deal  only  with  musical  tones,  that  is 
with  such  as  are  produced  by  exactly  periodic  motions  of  the  air,  and  exclude  all 
irregular  motions  of  the  air  which  appear  as  noises,  we  can  give  this  question  a 
more  definite  form.  If  we  suppose  the  motion  of  the  air  corresponding  to  the 
given  musical  tone  to  be  resolved  into  a  sum  of  pendular  vibrations  of  air,  such 
individual  pendular  vibrations  wiU  not  only  differ  from  each  other  in  force  or 
amplitude  for  different  forms  of  the  compound  motion,  but  also  in  their  relative 
position,  or,  according  to  physical  terminology,  in  their  difference  of  phase.  For 
example,  if  we  superimpose  the  two  pendular  vibrational  curves  A  and  B,  fig.  31 
(p.  i2oa),  first  with  the  point  e  of  B  on  the  point  do  of  A,  and  next  with  the  point 
e  of  B  on  the  point  d*  of  A^  we  obtain  the  two  entirely  distinct  vibrational  curves 

Digitized  by  V^OOQIC 


I20 


DOES  QUALITY  DEPEND  ON  PHASE? 


PART  I. 


C  and  D.  By  farther  displacement  of  the  initial  point  e  so  as  to  place  it  on  d^  or 
d,  we  obtain  other  forms,  which  are  the  inversions  of  the  forms  C  and  D,  as  has 
been  already  shewn  (supra,  p.  32a).  If,  then,  musical  quality  of  tone  depends  solely 
on  the  relative  force  of  the  partial  tones,  aU  the  various  motions  C,  D,  &c.,  must 


Fig.  31. 


B 


IT 


make  the  same  impression  on  the  ear.    But  if  the  relative  position  of  the  two 
^  waves,  that  is  the  difference  of  phase,  produces  any  effect,  they  must  make  different 
impressions  on  the  ear. 

Now  to  determine  this  point  it  was  necessary  to  compoxmd  various  musical 
tones  out  of  simple  tones  artificially,  and  to  see  whether  an  alteration  of  quality 
ensued  when  force  was  constant  but  phase  varied.  Simple  tones  of  great  purity, 
which  can  have  both  their  force  and  phase  exactly  regulated,  are  best  obtained 
from  tuning-forks  having  the  lowest  proper  tone  reinforced,  as  has  been  already 
described  (p.  54^^),  by  a  resonance  chamber,  and  communicated  to  the  air.  To  set 
the  tuning-forks  in  very  uniform  motion,  they  were  placed  between  the  limbs  of  a 
little  electro-magnet,  as  shewn  in  fig.  32,  opposite.  Each  tuning-fork  was  screwed 
mto  a  separate  board  d  d,  which  rested  upon  pieces  of  india-rubber  tubing  e  e  that 
were  cemented  below  it,  to  prevent  the  vibrations  of  the  fork  from  being  directly 
communicated  to  the  table  and  hence  becoming  audible.  The  limbs  b  b  of  the 
electro-magnet  are  surrounded  with  wire,  and  its  pole  f  is  directed  to  the  fork. 
f  There  are  two  clamp  screws  g  on  the  board  d  d  which  are  in  conductive  connection 
with  the  coils  of  the  electro-magnet,  and  serve  to  introduce  other  wires  which 
conduct  the  electric  current.  To  set  the  forks  in  strong  vibration  the  strength  of 
these  streams  must  alternate  periodically.  These  are  generated  by  a  separate 
apparatus  to  be  presently  described  (fig.  33,  p.  1226,  c). 

When  forks  thus  arranged  are  set  in  vibration,  very  little  indeed  of  their  tone 
is  heard,  because  they  have  so  little  means  of  communicating  their  vibrations  to 
the  surrounding  air  or  adjacent  soHds.  To  make  the  tone  strongly  audible,  the 
resonance  chamber  i,  which  has  been  previously  tuned  to  the  pitch  of  the  fork, 
must  be  brought  near  it.  This  resonance  chamber  is  fastened  to  another  board  k, 
which  slides  in  a  proper  groove  made  in  the  board  d  d,  and  thus  allows  its  opening 
to  be  brought  very  near  to  the  fork.  In  the  figure  the  resonance  chamber  is  shewn 
at  a  distance  from  the  fork  in  order  to  exhibit  the  separate  parts  distinctly ;  when 
in  use,  it  is  brought  as  close  as  possible  to  the  fork.  The  mouth  of  the  resonance 
chamber  can  be  closed  by  a  hd  1  attached  to  a  lever  m.    By  pulling  tlie  string  n 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


CHAP.  VI. 


ARTIFICIAL  VOWELS. 


121 


the  lid  is  withdrawn  from  the  opening  and  the  tone  of  the  fork  is  communicated 
to  the  air  with  great  force.  When  the  thread  is  let  loose,  the  lid  is  brought  over 
the  mouth  of  the  chamber  by  the  spring  p,  and  the  tone  of  the  fork  is  no  longer 
heard.  By  partial  opening  of  the  mouth  of  the  chamber,  the  tone  of  the  fork  can 
be  made  to  receive  any  desired  intermediate  degree  of  strength.  The  whole  of 
the  strings  which  open  the  various  resonance  chambers  belonging  to  a  series  of 
such  forks  are  attached  to  a  keyboard  in  such  a  way  that  by  pressing  a  key  the 
corresponding  chamber  is  opened. 

At  first  I  had  eight  forks  of  this  kind,  giving  the  tones  B{}  and  its  first  seven 
harmonic  upper  partials,  namely  l^,f,  b]},  d",  /',  a"[>,*  and  h"\}.  The  prime 
tone  I^  corresponds  to  the  pitch  in  which  bass  voices  naturally  speak.  Afterwards 
I  had  forks  made  of  the  pitches  d"',f"',  a'"b*  and  h'"\^,  and  assumed  l^  for  the 
prime  of  the  compound  tone. 

To  set  the  forks  in  motion,  intermittent  electrical  currents  had  to  be  conducted  H 
through  the  coils  of  the  electro-magnet,  giving  as  many  electrical  shocks  as  the 

FW.  32. 


f 


lowest  forks  made  vibrations  in  a  second,  namely  120.  Every  shock  makes  the 
iron  of  the  electro-magnet  b  b  momentarily  magnetic,  and  hence  enables  it  to 
attract  the  prongs  of  the  fork,  which  are  themselves  rendered  permanently  magnetic. 
The  prongs  of  the  lowest  fork  B^  are  thus  attracted  by  the  poles  of  the  electro- 1; 
magnet,  for  a  Very  short  time,  once  in  every  vibration  ;  the  prongs  of  the  second 
for  6|>,  which  moves  twice  as  fast,  once  every  second  vibration,  and  so  on.  The 
vibrations  of  the  forks  are  therefore  both  excited  and  kept  up  as  long  as  the  electric 
currents  pass  through  the  apparatus.  The  vibrations  of  the  lower  forks  are  very 
powerful,  those  of  the  higher  proportionally  weaker. 

The  apparatus  shewn  in  fig.  33  (p.  1226,  c)  serves  to  produce  intermittent  currents 
of  exactly  determinate  periodicity.  A  tuning-fork  a  is  fixed  horizontally  between 
the  limbs  b  b  of  an  electro-magnet ;  at  its  extremities  are  fastened  two  platinum 
wires  c  c,  which  dip  into  two  little  cups  d  filled  half  with  mercury  and  half  with 
alcohol,  forming  the  upper  extremities  of  brass  columns.  These  columns  have  clamp- 
ing screws  i  to  receive  the  wires,  and  stand  on  two  boards  f,  g,  which  turn  about 
an  axis,  as  at  f,  and  which  can  each  be  somewhat  raised  or  lowered  by  a  thumb- 

and  a'"b,  in  the  justly  intoned  scale  of  e\>, — 
Translator.] 

Digitized  by  V^ O OQ IC 


*  [These  being   7th  harmonics   Vb  and 
Vb    are    27    cents    flatter    than    the    a"\> 


122 


ARTIFICIAL  VOWELS. 


PABTI. 


screw,  as  at  g,  so  as  to  make  the  points  of  the  platinum  wires  c  c  exactly  toach 
the  mercury  below  the  alcohol  in  the  cups  d.  A  third  clamping  screw  e  is  in  con- 
ductive connection  with  the  handle  of  the  tuning-fork.  When  the  fork  vibrates, 
and  an  electric  current  passes  through  it  from  i  to  e,  the  current  will  be  broken 
every  time  that  the  end  of  the  fork  a  rises  above  the  surface  of  the  mercury  in  the 
cup  d,  and  re-made  every  time  the  platinum  wire  dips  again  into  the  mercury. 
This  intermittent  current  being  at  the  same  time  conducted  through  the  electro- 
magnet b  b,  fig.  33,  the  latter  becomes  magnetic  every  time  it  passes,  and  thus 
keeps  up  the  vibrations  of  the  fork  a,  which  is  itself  magnetic.  Generally  only 
one  of  the  cups  d  is  used  for  conducting  the  current.  Alcohol  is  poured  over  the 
mercury  to  prevent  the  latter  from  being  burned  by  the  electrical  sparks  which 
arise  when  the  stream  is  interrupted.  This  method  of  interrupting  the  current 
was  invented  by  Neef ,  who  used  a  simple  vibrating  spring  in  place  of  the  tamng- 
%  fork,  as  may  be  generally  seen  in  the  induction  apparatus  so  much  used  for  medical 
purposes.    But  the  vibrations  of  a  spring  communicate  themsedves  to  all  adjacent 

Fio.  33. 


bodies  and  are  for  our  purposes  both  too  audible  and  too  irregular.  Hence  the 
necessity  of  substituting  a  tuning-fork  for  the  spring.  The  handle  of  a  well  worked 
symmetrical  tuning-fork  is  extremely  little  agitated  by  the  vibrations  of  the  fork 
and  hence  does  not  itself  agitate  the  bodies  connected  with  it,  so  powerfully  as  the 
IT  fixed  end  of  a  straight  spring.  The  tuning-fork  of  the  apparatus  in  fig.  33  must 
be  in  exact  unison  with  the  prime  tone  B\}.  To  effect  this  I  employ  a  little  clamp 
of  thick  steel  wire  h,  placed  on  one  of  the  prongs.  By  slipping  this  towards  the 
free  end  of  the  prong  the  tone  is  deepened,  and  by  shpping  it  towards  the  handle 
of  the  fork,  the  tone  is  raised.* 

When  the  whole  apparatus  is  in  action,  but  the  resonance  chambers  are  closed, 
all  the  forks  are  maintained  in  a  state  of  uniform  motion,  but  no  sound  is  heard, 
beyond  a  gentle  humming  caused  by  the  direct  action  of  the  forks  on  the  air.  But 
on  opening  one  or  more  resonance  chambers,  the  corresponding  tones  are  heard 
with  sufficient  loudness,  and  are  louder  as  the  hd  is  more  widely  opened.  By  this 
means  it  is  possible  to  form,  in  rapid  succession,  different  combinations  of  the  prime 


♦  This  apparatas  was  made  by  Fesael  in  in  Appendix  VIII.  [This  apparatus  was  ex- 
Cologne.  More  detailed  descriptions  of  its  hibited  by  R.  Koenig  (see  Appendix  II.)  in  the 
separate  parts,  and  instructions  for  the  ex-  International  Exhibition  of  1872  in  London, 
periments  to  be  made  by  its  means,  are  given  — Translator.] 


Digitized  by  V^ O OQ IC 


CHAP.  VI.  ARTIFICIAL  VOWELS.  123 

tone  with  one  or  more  harmonic  upper  partials  having  different  degrees  of  loudness, 
and  thus  produoe  tones  of  different  qualities. 

Among  the  natural  musical  tones  which  appear  suitable  for  imitation  with  forks, 
the  vowels  of  the  human  voice  hold  the  first  rank,  because  they  are  accompanied  by 
comparatively  little  extraneous  noise  and  shew  distinct  differences  of  quality  which 
are  easy  to  seize.  Most  vowels  also  are  characterised  by  comparatively  low  upper 
partials,  which  can  be  reached  by  our  forks ;  E  and  I  alone  somewhat  exceed  these 
limits.  The  motion  of  the  very  high  forks  is  too  weak  for  this  purpose  when  in- 
fluenced only  by  such  electrical  currents  as  I  was  able  to  use  without  disturbance 
from  the  noise  of  the  electric  sparks. 

The  first  series  of  experiments  was  made  with  the  eight  forks  S\}  to  b']}.  With 
these  U,  0,  0,  and  even  A  could  be  imitated ;  the  last  not  very  well  because  of  my 
not  possessing  the  upper  partials  &"  and  d"\  which  lie  immediately  above  its 
characteristic  tone  ^'l^,  and  are  sensibly  reinforced  in  the  natural  sound  of  this  f 
vowel.  The  prime  tone  JB\}  of  this  series,  when  sounded  alone,  gave  a  very  dull 
U,  much  duller  than  could  be  produced  in  speech.  The  sound  became  more  like 
U  when  the  second  and  third  partial  tones  l^  and/'  were  allowed  to  sound  feebly 
at  the  same  time.  A  very  fine  0  was  produced  by  taking  V\}  strong,  and  £(>,  /',  d'^ 
more  feebly ;  the  prime  tone  ^  had  then,  however,  to  be  somewhat  damped.  On 
Budteily  changing  the  pressure  on  the  keys  and  hence  the  position  of  the  hds 
before  the  resonance  chambers,  so  as  to  give  B[}  strong,  and  all  the  upper  partials 
weak,  the  apparatus  uttered  a  good  dear  U  after  the  0. 

A  or  rather  A®  [nearly  0  in  not]  was  produced  by  making  the  fifth  to  the  eighth 
partial  tones  as  loud  as  possible,  and  keeping  the  rest  under. 

The  vowels  of  the  second  and  third  series,  which  have  higher  characteristic  tones, 
could  be  only  imperfectly  imitated  by  bringing  out  their  reinforced  tones  of  the  lower 
pitch.  Though  not  very  clear  in  themselves  they  became  so  by  contrast  on  alterna- 
tion with  U  and  0.  Thus  a  passably  clear  A  was  obtained  by  giving  loudness  f 
chiefly  to  the  fourth  and  fifth  tones,  and  keeping  down  the  lower  ones,  and  a  sort 
of  E  by  reinforcing  the  third,  and  letting  the  rest  sound  feebly.  The  difference 
between  0  and  these  two  vowels  lay  principally  in  keeping  the  prime  tone  Bj[}  and 
its  Octave  l\}  much  weaker  for  A  and  E  than  for  0.* 

To  extend  my  experiments  to  the  brighter  vowels,  I  afterwards  added  the  forks 
^"'»/"»  ^'"b>  ^'"b»  ^^^  ^^^  upper  ones  of  which,  however,  gave  a  very  fidnt  tone, 
and  I  chose  h\}  as  the  prime  tone  in  place  of  ^.  With  these  I  got  a  very  good  A 
and  A,  and  at  least  a  much  more  distinct  E  than  before.  But  I  could  not  get  up 
to  the  high  characteristic  tone  of  I. 

In  this  higher  series  of  forks,  the  prime  tone  5t>,  when  sounded  alone,  repro- 
duced U.  The  same  prime  5t>  with  moderate  force,  accompanied  with  a  strong 
Octave  6t>,  and  a  weaker  Twelfth  /",  gave  0,  which  has  the  characteristic  tone  6t>. 
A  was  obtained  by  taking  5t>,  b'[},  and  f'  moderately  strong,  and  the  characteristic 
tones  b"\}  and  d'"  very  strong.  To  change  A  into  A  it  was  necessary  to  increase  % 
somewhat  the  force  of  b^  and/''  which  were  adjacent  to  the  characteristic  tone 
d",  to  damp  b'%  and  bring  out  (i'"  and/"  as  strongly  as  possible.  For  E  the  two 
deepest  tones  of  the  series,  bj;}  and  b'\},  had  to  be  kept  moderately  loud,  as  being 
adjacent  to  the  deeper  characteristic  tone/',  while  the  highest/''',  a"'b,  &'"[>  had 
to  be  made  as  prominent  as  possible.  But  I  have  hitherto  not  succeeded  so  well 
with  this  as  with  the  other  vowels,  because  the  high  forks  were  too  weak,  and 
because  perhaps  the  upper  partials  which  he  above  the  characteristic  tone  6"'[> 
could  not  be  entirely  dispensed  with.f 

*  The  statementB  in  the  MUnchener  gelehrte  above  results  will  serve  to  shew  their  relations 

Anzeigen  for  June  20,  1859,  mast  be  corrected  more  clearly.    In  the  first  line  are  placed  the 

accordingly.    At  that  time  I  did  not  know  the  notes  of  the  forks  and  the  numbers  of  the 

higher  upper  partials  of  £  and  I,  and  hence  corresponding  partials.    The  letters  pp,  p,  mf, 

made  the  O  too  dull  to  distinguish  it  from  the  /,  ff  below  them  are  the  usual  musical  indica- 

unperfect  E.  tions  of  force,  pianissiino^  piano,  mezzo  forte^ 

t  [The  following  tabular  statement  of  the  forte^  fortissimo.     Where  no  such  mark  is 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


124 


QUALITY  INDEPENDENT  OF  PHASE. 


FABT  I. 


In  precisely  the  same  way  as  the  vowels  of  the  human  voice,  it  is  possible  to 
imitate  the  quality  of  tone  produced  by  organ  pipes  of  different  stops,  if  they  have 
not  secondary  tones  which  are  too  high,  but  of  course  the  whizzing  noise,  formed 
by  breaking  the  stream  of  air  at  the  lip,  is  wanting  in  these  imitations.  The 
tuning-forks  are  necessarily  limited  to  the  imitation  of  the  purely  musical  part  of 
the  tone.  The  piercing  high  upper  partials,  required  for  imitating  reed  instru- 
ments, were  absent,  but  the  nasahty  of  the  clarinet  was  given  by  using  a  series 
of  unevenly  numbered  partials,  and  the  softer  tones  of  the  horn  by  the  full  chorus 
of  all  the  forks. 

But  though  it  was  not  possible  to  imitate  every  kind  of  quality  of  tone  by  the 
present  apparatus,  it  sufficed  to  decide  the  important  question  as  to  the  effect  of 
altered  difference  of  phase  upon  quality  of  tone.  As  I  particularly  observed  at  the 
beginning  of  this  chapter,  this  question  is  of  fundamental  importance  for  the 

IT  theory  of  auditory  sensation.  The  reabder  who  is  unused  to  physical  investigations 
must  excuse  some  apparently  dif&cult  and  dry  details  in  the  explanation  of  the 
experiments  necessary  for  its  decision. 

The  simple  means  of  altering  the  phases  of  the  secondary  tones  consists  in 
bringing  the  resonance  chambers  somewhat  out  of  tune  by  narrowing  their 
apertures,  which  weakens  the  resonance,  and  at  the  same  time  alters  the  phase. 
If  the  resonance  chamber  is  tuned  so  that  the  simple  tone  which  excites  its 
strongest  resonance  coincides  with  the  simple  tone  of  the  corresponding  fork,  then, 
as  the  mathematical  theory  shews,*  the  greatest  velocity  of  the  air  at  the  mouth 
of  the  chamber  in  an  outward  direction,  coincides  with  the  greatest  velocity  of  the 
ends  of  the  fork  in  an  inward  direction.  On  the  other  hand,  if  the  chamber  is 
tuned  to  be  slightly  deeper  than  the  fork,  the  greatest  velocity  of  the  air  slightly 
precedes,  and  if  it  is  timed  slightly  higher,  that  greatest  velocity  slightly  lags 
behind  the  greatest  velocity  of  the  fork.    The  more  the  tuning  is  altered,  the 

^  greater  will  be  the  difference  of  phase,  tiU  at  last  it  reaches  the  duration  of  a 
quarter  of  a  vibration.  The  magnitude  of  the  difference  of  phase  agrees  during 
this  change  precisely  with  the  strength  of  the  resonance,  so  that  to  a  certain  degree 
we  are  able  to  measure  the  former  by  the  latter.  If  we  represent  the  strength  of 
the  sound  in  the  resonance  chamber  when  in  unison  with  the  fork  by  lo,  and 
divide  the  periodic  time  of  a  vibration,  like  the  circumference  of  a  circle,  into  360 

added  the  partial  is  not  mentioned  in  the  text,      ones,  but  the  whole  are  now  numbered  as  par- 
For  the  second  series  of  experiments  the  forks      tials  of  6  b. 
of  corresponding  pitches  are  kept  under  the  old 


f   -I 


Firet   ) 
Forks)- 

z 

a 

}' 

^\ 

!'' 

6 

ah 

8 

lO 

xa 

»4 

16 

u 

f 

PP 

PP 

.3 

0 

mf 

P 

P 

f 

P 

^ 

A° 

P 

P 

P 

P 

ff 

// 

ff 

ff 

> 

A 

P 

PP 

? 

f 

f 

£ 

P 

PP 

f 

P 

P 

P 

P 

P 

Second) 
Forks   r 

X 

a 

/" 

A 

i- 

6 

ah 

2 

U 

f 

% 

0 

mf 

f 

P 

1 

A 
A 
E 

mf 
mf 

mf 

mf 

f 
P 

/ 

f, 

ff 

ff 

See  Appendix  XX.  sect.  M.  No.  2,  for 
Messrs.  Preece  and  Stroh's  new  method  of 
vowel  synthesis. — Translator.l 


See  the  first  part  of  Appendix  IX. 


Digitized  by 


Google 


CHAP.  VI. 


QUALITY  INDEPENDENT  OF  PHASE. 


125 


degrees,  the  relation  between  the  strength  of  the  resonance  and  the  difference  of 
phase  is  shewn  by  the  following  table  : — 


strength  of. 

Difference  of  Phase  in  angular 

Beeonance 

degrees 

10 

o<^ 

9 

35*^  54' 

8 

SO""  12' 

7 

60*^40' 

6 

68<'  54' 

5 

75°  31' 

4 

8o<>48' 

3 

84°  50' 

2 

87°  42' 

I 

89°  26' 

This  table  shews  that  a  comparatively  slight  weakening  of  resonance  by 
altering  the  timing  of  the  chamber  occasions  considerable  differences  of  phase, 
but  that  when  the  weakening  is  considerable  there  are  relatively  slight  changes 
of  phase.  We  can  take  advantage  of  this  circmnstance  when  compomiding  the 
vowel  somids  by  means  of  the  tmiing-forks  to  produce  every  possible  alteration  of 
phase.  It  is  only  necessary  to  let  the  lid  shade  the  mouth  of  the  resonance 
chamber  till  the  strength  of  the  tone  is  perceptibly  diminished.  As  soon  as  we 
have  learned  how  to  estimate  roughly  the  amount  of  diminution  of  loudness,  the 
above  table  gives  us  the  corresponding  alteration  of  phase.  We  are  thus  able  to 
alter  the  vibrations  of  the  tones  in  question  to  any  amount,  up  to  a  quarter  of  the 
periodic  time  of  a  vibration.  Alterations  of  phase  to  the  amount  of  half  the 
periodic  time  are  produced  by  sending  the  electric  current  through  the  electro- 
magnets of  the  corresponding  fork  in  an  opposite  direction,  which  causes  the  ends 
of  the  fork  to  be  repelled  instead  of  attracted  by  the  electro-magnets  on  the  ^ 
passage  of  the  current,  and  thus  sets  the  fork  vibrating  in  the  contrary  direction. 
This  counter-excitement  of  the  fork,  however,  by  repelling  currents,  must  not  be 
continued  too  long,  as  the  magnetism  of  the  fork  itself  would  otherwise  gradually 
diminish,  whereas  attracting  currents  strengthen  it  or  maintain  it  at  a  maximum. 
It  is  well  known  that  the  magnetism  of  masses  of  iron  that  are  violently  agitated 
is  easily  altered. 

After  a  tone  has  been  compounded,  in  which  some  of  the  partials  have  been 
weakened  and  at  the  same  time  altered  in  phase  by  the  half-shading  of  the 
apertures  of  their  corresponding  resonance  chambers,  we  can  re-compound  the 
same  tone  by  an  equal  amount  of  weakening  in  the  same  partials,  but  without 
shading  the  aperture,  and  therefore  without  change  of  phase,  by  simply  leaving 
the  months  of  the  chambers  wide  open,  and  increasing  their  distances  from  the 
exciting  forks,  until  the  required  amount  of  enfeeblement  of  sound  is  attained. 

For  example,  let  us  first  sound  the  forks  Bj;}  and  Vp,  with  fully  opened  resonance  % 
chambers,  and  perfect  accord.  They  will  vibrate  as  shewn  by  the  vibrational 
forms  fig.  31,  A  and  B  (p.  120a),  with  the  points  e  and  do  coincident,  and  produce 
at  a  distance  the  compound  vibration  represented  by  the  vibrational  curve  C.  But 
by  closing  the  resonance  chamber  of  the  fork  Bj;}  we  can  make  the  point  e  on  the 
curve  B  coincide  with  the  points  between  d©  and  dj  on  the  curve  A.  To  make  e 
coincide  with  d,,  the  loudness  of  B[}  must  be  made  about  three-quarters  of  what 
it  would  be  if  the  mouth  of  the  chamber  were  unshaded.  The  point  e  can  be  made 
to  coincide  with  d4  by  reversing  the  current  in  the  electro-magnets  and  folly 
opening  the  mouth  of  the  resonance  chamber ;  and  then  by  imperfectly  opening 
the  chamber  of  J3|;>  the  point  e  can  be  made  to  move  towards  8.  On  the  other 
hand,  an  imperfect  opening  of  the  chamber  h\}  will  make  e  recede  from  coincidence 
with  8  (which  is  the  same  thing  as  coincidence  with  do)  or  with  d4 ,  towards  d4  or 
dj  respectively.    The  proportions  of  loudness  may  be  made  the  same  in  all  these 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


126  QUALITY  INDEPENDENT  OF  PHASE.  pakt  i. 

cases,  without  any  alteration  of  phase,  by  removing  the  corresponding  chambers 
to  the  proper  distance  from  its  forks  without  shading  its  mouth. 

In  this  manner  every  possible  difference  of  phase  in  the  tones  of  two  chambers 
can  be  produced.    The  same  process  can  of  course  be  applied  to  any  required 
number  of  forks.    I  have  thus  experimented  upon  numerous  combinations  of  tone 
with  varied  differences  of  phase,  and  I  have  never  experienced  the  slightest  dif- 
ference in  the  quality  of  tone.     So  far  as  the  quality  of  tone  was  concerned,  I 
found  that  it  was  entirely  indifferent  whether  I  weakened  the  separate  partial 
tones  by  shading  the  mouths  of  their  resonance  chambers,  or  by  moving  the 
chamber  itself  to  a  sufficient  distance  &om  the  fork.    Hence  the  answer  to  the 
proposed  question  is:   the  quality  of  the  musical  portion  of  a  compound  tone 
depends  solely  on  the  nv/nvher  and  relative  strength  of  its  partial  simple  tones^ 
and  in  no  respect  on  their  differences  of  phase.* 
f       The  preceding  proof  that  quality  of  tone  is  independent  of  difference  of 
phase,  is  the  easiest  to  carry  out  experimentally,  but  its  force  lies  solely  in  the 
theoretical  proposition  that  phases  alter  contemporaneously  with  strength  of  tone 
.  V      when  the  mouths  of  the  resonance  chambers  are  shaded,  and  this  proposition  is 
%^^^S»»^^j  the  result  of  mathematical  theory  alone.    We  cannot  make  vibrations  of  air 
^  directly  visible.    But  by  a  slight  change  in  the  experiment  it  may  be  so  conducted 

as  to  make  the  alteration  of  phase  immediately  visible.  It  is  only  necessary  to 
put  the  tuning-forks  themselves  out  of  tune  with  their  resonance  chambers,  by 
attaching  little  lumps  of  wax  to  the  prongs.  The  same  law  holds  for  the  phases 
of  a  tuning-fork  kept  in  vibration  by  an  electric  current,  as  for  the  resonance 
chambers  themselves.  The  phase  gradually  alters  by  a  quarter  period,  while  the 
strength  of  the  tone  of  the  fork  is  reduced  from  a  maximum  to  nothing  at  all,  by 
putting  it  out  of  tune.  The  phase  of  the  motion  of  the  air  retains  the  same 
relation  to  the  phase  of  the  vibration  of  the  fork,  because  the  pitch,  which  is 
f  determined  by  the  number  of  interruptions  of  the  electrical  current  in  a  second,  is 
not  altered  by  the  alteration  of  the  fork.  The  change  of  phase  in  the  fork  can  be 
observed  directly  by  means  of  Lissajou's  vibration  microscope,  already  described 
and  shewn  in  fig.  22  (p.  Sod).  Place  the  prongs  of  the  fork  and  the  microscope  of 
this  instrument  horizontally,  and  the  fork  to  be  examined  vertically  ;  powder  the 
upper  end  of  one  of  its  prongs  with  a  little  starch,  direct  the  microscope  to  one  of 
the  grains  of  starch,  and  excite  both  forks  by  means  of  the  electrical  currents  of 
the  interrupting  fork  (fig.  33,  p.  122b).  The  fork  of  Lissajou's  instrument  is  in 
unison  with  the  interrupting  fork.  The  grain  of  starch  vibrates  horizontally,  the 
object-glass  of  the  microscope  vertically,  and  thus,  by  the  composition  of  these 
two  motions,  curves  are  generated,  just  as  in  the  observations  on  violin  strings 
previously  described. 

When  the  observed  fork  is  in  unison  with  the  interrupting  fork,  the  curve 
becomes  an  obHque  straight  Une  (fig.  34,  i),  if  both  forks  pass  through  their 

%  RG 


VI 


position  of  rest  at  the  same  moment.  As  the  phase  alters,  the  straight  line  passes 
through  a  long  oblique  ellipse  (2,  3),  tiU  on  the  difference  of  phase  becoming  a 
quarter  of  a  period,  it  develops  into  a  circle  (4) ;  and  then  as  the  difference  of 
phase  increases,  it  passes  through  oblique  ellipses  (5,  6)  in  another  direction,  till  it 
reaches  another  straight  Une  (7),  on  the  difference  becoming  half  a  period. 

If  the  second  fork  is  the  upper  Octave  of  the  interrupting  fork,  the  curves 

*  [The  experiments  of  Koenig  with  the  modification.  Moreover  Koenig  contends  that 
wave-siren,  explained  in  App.  XX.  sect.  L.  the  'apparent  exception'  of  p.  127c,  is  an 
art.  6,  shew  that  this  law  requires  a  slight      '  actual '  one  (ibid.), — Translator.] 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


CHAP.  VI.  QUALITY  INDEPENDENT  OP  PHASE.  127 

i»  ^f  3»  4»  5>  ui  fig.  35,  shew  the  series  of  forms.  Here  3  answers  to  the  case  when 
both  forks  pass  through  their  position  of  rest  at  the  same  time  ;  2  and  4  differ  from 
that  position  by  ^,  and  i  and  5  by  ^  of  a  wave  of  the  higher  fork. 

If  we  now  bring  the  forks  into  the  most  perfect  possible  unison  with  the 
interrupting  fork,  so  that  both  vibrate  as  strongly  as  possible,  and  then  alter  their 


timing  a  little  by  putting  on  or  removing  pieces  of  wax,  we  also  see  one  figure  of  the 
microscopic  image  gradually  passing  into  another,  and  can  thus  easily  assure  our-  f 
selves  of  the  correctness  of  the  law  already  cited.  Experiments  on  quality  of  tone 
are  then  conducted  by  first  bringing  all  the  forks  as  exactly  as  possible  to  the 
pitches  of  the  harmonic  upper  partial  tones  of  the  interrupting  fork,  next  removing 
the  resonance  chambers  to  such  distances  from  the  forks  as  will  give  the  required 
relations  of  strength,  and  finally  putting  the  forks  out  of  tune  as  much  as  we  please 
by  sticking  on  lumps  of  wax.  The  size  of  these  lumps  should  be  previously  so 
regulated  by  microscopical  observation  as  to  produce  the  required  difference  of 
phase.  This,  however,  at  the  same  time  weakens  the  vibrations  of  the  forks,  and 
hence  the  strength  of  the  tones  must  be  restored  to  its  former  state  by  bringing  the 
resonance  chambers  nearer  to  the  forks.  u 

The  result  in  these  experiments,  where  the  forks  are  put  out  of  tune,  is  the 
same  as  in  those  where  the  resonance  chambers  were  put  out  of  tune.  There  is 
no  perceptible  alteration  of  quality  of  tone.  At  least  there  is  no  alteration  so 
marked  as  to  be  recognisable  after  the  expiration  of  the  few  seconds  necessary  % 
for  resetting  the  apparatus,  and  hence  certainly  no  such  change  of  quality  as 
would  change  one  vowel  into  another. 

An  apparent  exception  to  this  rule  must  here  be  mentioned.  If  the  forks  JB\} 
and  H}  are  not  perfectly  tuned  as  Octaves,  and  are  brought  into  vibration  by  rub- 
bing or  striking,  an  attentive  ear  will  observe  very  weak  beats  which  appear  like 
small  changes  in  the  strength  of  the  tone  and  its  quality.  These  beats  are  cer- 
tainly connected  with  the  successive  entrance  of  the  vibrating  forks  on  varying 
difference  of  phase.  Their  explanation  will  be  given  when  combinational  tones  are 
considered,  and  it  will  then  be  shewn  that  these  slight  variations  of  quality  are 
referable  to  changes  in  the  strength  of  one  of  the  simple  tones. 

Hence  we  are  able  to  lay  down  the  important  law  that  differences  in  musical 
quaiity  of  tone  depend  solely  on  the  presence  and  strength  of  partial  tones,  and  in 
no  respect  on  the  differences  in  phase  under  which  these  partial  tones  enter  into 
composition.  It  must  be  here  observed  that  we  are  speaking  only  of  musical  ^ 
quality  as  previously  defined.  When  the  musical  tone  is  accompanied  by  un- 
musical noises,  such  as  jarring,  scratching,  soughing,  whizzing,  hissing,  these 
motions  are  either  not  to  be  considered  as  periodic  at  all,  or  else  correspond  to 
high  upper  partials,  of  nearly  the  same  pitch,  which  consequently  form  strident 
dissonances.  We  were  not  able  to  embrace  these  in  our  experiments,  and  hence 
we  must  leave  it  for  the  present  doubtful  whether  in  such  dissonating  tones 
difference  of  phase  is  an  element  of  importance.  Subsequent  theoretic  considera- 
tions will  lead  us  to  suppose  that  it  really  is. 

If  we  wish  only  to  imitate  vowels  by  compound  tones  without  being  able  to 
distinguish  the  differences  of  phase  in  the  individual  constituent  simple  tones,  we 
can  effect  our  purpose  tolerably  well  with  organ  pipes.  But  we  must  have  at  least 
two  series  of  them,  loud  open  and  soft  stopped  pipes,  because  the  strength  of  tone 
cannot  be  increased  by  additional  pressure  of  wind  without  at  the  same  time 
changing  the  pitch.     I  have  had  a  double  row  of  pipes  of  this  kind  made  by  Herr 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


128 


APPEEHENSION  OF  QUALITY  OP  TONE. 


PART  I. 


A 


Appunn  in  Hanau,  giving  the  first  sixteen  partial  tones  of  B\}.  All  these  pipes 
stand  on  a  common  windchest,  which  also  contains  the  valves  by  which  they  can 
be  opened  or  shut.  Two  larger  valves  cut  off  the  passage  from  the  windchest  to 
the  bellows.  While  these  valves  are  closed,  the  pipe  valves  are  arranged  for  the 
required  combination  of  tones,  and  then  one  of  the  main  valves  of  the  windchest 
is  opened,  allowing  all  the  pipes  to  sound  at  once.  The  character  of  the  vowel  is 
better  produced  in  this  way  by  short  jerks  of  sound,  than  by  a  long  continued 
sound.  It  is  best  to  produce  the  prime  tone  and  the  predominant  upper  partial 
tones  of  the  required  vowels  on  both  the  open  and  stopped  pipes  at  once,  and  to 
open  only  the  weak  stopped  pipes  for  the  next  adjacent  tones,  so  that  the  strong 
tone  may  not  be  too  isolated. 

The  imitation  of  the  vowels  by  this  means  is  not  very  perfect,  because,  among 
other  reasons,  it  is  impossible  to  graduate  the  strength  of  tone  on  the  different  pipes 

^  so  delicately  as  on  the  tuning-forks,  and  the  higher  tones  especially  are  too  scream- 
ing.   But  the  vowel  sounds  thus  composed  are  perfectly  recognisable. 

We  proceed  now  to  consider  the  part  played  by  the  ear  in  the  apprehension  of 
quality  of  tone.  The  assumption  formerly  made  respecting  the  function  of  the  ear, 
was  that  it  was  capable  of  distinguishing  both  the  pitch  number  of  a  musical  tone 
(which  gives  the  pitch),  and  also  the  /orm  of  the  vibrations  (on  which  the  difference 
of  quality  depends).  This  last  assertion  was  based  simply  on  the  exclusion  of  all 
other  possible  assumptions.  As  it  could  be  proved  that  sameness  of  pitch  always 
required  equal  pitch  numbers,  and  as  loudness  visibly  depended  upon  the  ampli- 
tude of  the  vibrations,  the  quality  of  tone  must  necessarily  depend  on  something 
which  was  neither  the  number  nor  the  amplitude  of  the  vibrations.  There  was 
nothing  left  us  but  form.  We  can  now  make  this  view  more  definite.  The  ex- 
periments just  described  shew  that  waves  of  very  different  forms  (as  fig.  31, 
0,  D,  p.  1 20a,  and  fig.  12,  C,  D,  p.  22b),  may  have  the  same  quality  of  tone,  and 

m  indeed,  for  every  case,  except  the  simple  tone,  there  is  an  infinite  number  of  forms 
of  wave  of  this  kind,  because  any  alteration  of  the  difference  of  phase  alters  the 
form  of  wave  without  changing  the  quality  of  tone.  The  only  decisive  character 
of  a  quality  of  tone,  is  that  the  motion  of  the  air  which  strikes  the  ear  when  re- 
solved into  a  sum  of  pendulum  vibrations  gives  the  same  degree  of  strength  to  the 
^ame  simple  vibration. 

'      Hence  the  ear  does  not  distinguish  the  different  forms  of  waves  in  themselves, 
/as  the  eye  distinguishes  the  different  vibrational  curves.    The  ear  must  be  said 

/  rather  to  decompose  every  wave  form  into  simpler  elements  according  to  a  definite 

^^^'W.  It  then  receives  a  sensation  from  each  of  these  simpler  elements  as  from  an 
harmonious  tone.  By  trained  attention  the  ear  is  able  to  become  conscious  of  each 
of  these  simpler  tones  separately.  And  what  the  ear  distinguishes  as  different 
qualities  of  tone  are  only  different  combinations  of  these  simpler  sensations. 

The  comparison  between  ear  and  eye  is  here  very  instructive.    When  the 

«r  vibrational  motion  is  rendered  visible,  as  in  the  vibration  microscope,  the  eye  is 
capable  of  distinguishing  every  possible  different  form  of  vibration  one  from 
another,  even  such  as  the  ear  cannot  distinguish.  But  the  eye  is  not  capable  of 
directly  resolving  the  vibrations  into  simple  vibrations,  as  the  ear  is.  Hence  the 
eye,  assisted  by  the  above-named  instrument,  really  distinguishes  the  form  ofvibra- 
tioUy  as  such,  and  in  so  doing  distinguishes  every  different  form  of  vibration.  The 
ear,  on  the  other  hand,  does  Tiot  distinguish  every  different  form  of  vibration,  but 
only  such  as  when  resolved  into  pendular  vibrations,  give  different  constituents. 
But  on  the  other  hand,  by  its  capability  of  distinguishing  and  feeling  these  very 
constituents,  it  is  again  superior  to  the  eye,  which  is  quite  incapable  of  so  doing. 

This  analysis  of  compound  into  simple  pendular  vibrations  is  an  astonishing 
property  of  the  ear.  The  reader  must  bear  in  mind  that  when  we  apply  the  term 
*  compound '  to  the  vibrations  produced  by  a  single  musical  instrument,  the  *  com- 
position '  has  no  existence  except  for  our  auditory  perceptions,  or  for  mathematical 
theory.    In  reality,  the  motion  of  the  particles  of  the  air  is  not  at  all  compound. 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


CHAP.  VI.   SYMPATHETICALLY  VIBKATING  PARTS  OF  THE  EAR.    129 


it  is  quite  simple,  flowing  from  a  single  source.  When  we  turn  to  external  nature 
for  an  analogue  of  such  an  analysis  of  periodical  motions  into  simple  motions,  we 
find  none  but  the  phenomena  of  sympathetic  vibration.  Li  reality  if  we  suppose 
the  dampers  of  a  pianoforte  to  be  raised,  and  allow  any  musical  tone  to  impinge 
powerfully  on  its  sounding  board,  we  bring  a  set  of  strings  into  sympathetic  vibra- 
tion, namely  all  those  strings,  and  only  those,  which  correspond  with  the  simple 
tones  contained  in  the  given  musical  tone.  Here,  then,  we  have,  by  a  purely  me- 
chanical process,  a  resolution  of  air  waves  precisely  similar  to  that  performed  by  the 
ear.  The  air  wave,  quite  simple  in  itself,  brings  a  certain  number  of  strings  into 
sympathetic  vibration,  and  the  sympathetic  vibration  of  these  strings  depends  on 
the  same  law  as  the  sensation  of  harmcHiic  upper  partial  tones  in  the  ear.* 

There  is  necessarily  a  certain  difference  between  the  two  kinds  of  apparatus, 
because  the  pianoforte  strings  readily  vibrate  with  their  upper  partials  in  S3nnpathy, 
and  hence  separate  into  several  vibrating  sections.  We  will  disregard  this  pecu-  ^ 
liarity  in  making  our  comparison.  It  would  besides  be  easy  to  make  an  instrument 
in  which  the  strings  would  not  vibrate  sensibly  or  powerfully  for  any  but  their 
prime  tones,  by  simply  loading  the  strings  slightly  in  the  middle.  This  would  make 
their  higher  proper  tones  inharmonic  to  their  primes. 

Now  suppose  we  were  able  to  connect  every  string  of  a  piano  with  a  nervous  fibre 
in  such  a  manner  that  this  fibre  would  be  excited  and  experience  a  sensation  every 
time  the  string  vibrated.  Then  every  musical  tone  which  impinged  on  the  instru- 
ment would  excite,  as  we  know  to  be  really  the  case  in  the  ear,  a  series  of  sensa- 
tions exactly  corresponding  to  the  pendular  vibrations  into  which  the  original 
motion  of  the  air  had  to  be  resolved.  By  this  means,  then,  the  existence  of  each 
partial  tone  would  be  exactly  so  perceived,  as  it  really  is  perceived  by  the  ear. 
The  sensations  of  simple  tones  of  different  pitch  would  under  the  supposed  con- 
ditions fisll  to  the  lot  of  different  nervous  fibres,  and  hence  be  produced  quite 
separately,  and  independently  of  each  other.  ^ 

Now,  as  a  matter  of  fact,  later  microscopic  discoveries  respecting  the  internal 
construction  of  the  ear,  lead  to  the  hypothesis,  that  arrangements  exist  in  the  ear 

similar  to  those  which  we 
have  imagined.  The  end  of 
every  fibre  of  the  auditory 
nerve  is  connected  with  small 
elastic  parts,  which  we  cannot 
but  assume  to  be  set  in  sym- 
pathetic vibration  by  the 
waves  of  sound. 

The  construction  of  the 
ear  may  be  briefly  described 
as  follows: — The  fine   ends 
of  the  fibres  of  the  auditory  ^ 
nerves  are  expanded  on  a  deli- 
cate membrane  in   a  cavity 
filled  with  fluid.     Owing  to 
its  involved  form  this  cavity 
is  known  as  the  labyrinth  of  the  ear.     To  conduct  the  vibrations  of  the  air  with 
sufficient  force  into  the  fluid  of  the  labyrinth  is  the  office  of  a  second  portion  of 
the  ear,  the  tympdrmm  or  drum  and  the  parts  within  it.    Fig.   36  above  is  a 

^  [Baise  the  dampers  of  a  piano,  and  utter 
the  Yowel  A  (ah)  sharply  and  loudly,  directing  it 
well  on  to  the  sound  board,  pause  a  second  and 
the  vowel  will  be  echoed  from  the  strings.    Be- 


^h 


Fig.  36. 


damp,  raise  the  dampers  and  cry  U  {00)  as  be- 
fore, and  that  will  also  be  echoed.  He-damp, 
raise  the  dampers  and  cry  I  (ee),  and  that 
again  will  be  echoed.  The  other  vowels  may 
be  tried  in  the  same  way.     The  echo,  though 


imperfect,  is  always  true  enough  to  surprise 
a  hearer  to  whom  it  is  new,  even  if  the  pitch  of 
the  vowel  is  taken  at  hazard.  It  will  be  im- 
proved if  the  vowels  are  sung  loudly  to  notes 
of  the  piano.  The  experiment  is  so  easy  to 
make  and  so  fundamental  in  character,  that 
it  should  be  witnessed  by  every  student.  - 
Translator.] 


Digitized  by 


Google 


130     SYMPATHETICALLY  VIBRATING  PARTS  OF  THE   EAR.   parti. 


diagrammatic  section,  of  the  size  of  life,  shewing  the  cavities  belonging  to  the 
auditory  apparatus.  A  is  the  labyrinth,  B  B  the  cavity  of  the  tympdwwm  or  drum, 
D  the  funnel-shaped  entrance  into  the  meatus  or  external  auditory  passage,  nar- 
rowest in  the  middle  and  expanding  slightly  towards  its  upper  extremity.  This 
medtiis,  in  the  ear  or  passage,  is  a  tube  formed  partly  of  cartilage  or  gristle  and 
partly  of  bone,  and  it  is  separated  &om  the  tymp&num  or  drum,  by  a  thin  circular 
membrane,  the  membrdna  tymp&nl  or  d/mmshin*  c  c,  which  is  rather  laxly  stretched 
in  a  bony  ring.  The  drum  (tym'p&num)  B  lies  between  the  outer  passage 
(meatus)  and  the  labyrinth.  Th^  drum  is  separated  from  the  labyrinth  by  bony 
walls,  pierced  with  two  holes,  closed  by  membranes.  These  are  the  so-called 
windows  [fenes'trae)  of  the  labyrinth.  The  upper  one  o,  called  the  oval  window 
(fenes'tra  dvdUs),  is  connected  with  one  of  the  ossicles  or  little  bones  of  the  ear 
called  the  stirrup.    The  lower  or  round  window  r  (fenes'tra  rotun'da)  has  no 

%  connection  with  these  ossicles. 

The  drum  of  the  ear  is  consequently  completely  shut  off  from  the  external 
passage  and  from  the  labyrinth.  But  it  has  free  access  to  the  upper  part  of  the 
pharynx  or  throat,  through  the  so-called  Eustachianf  tube  E,  which  in  Oermany 
is  termed  a  trumpet,  because  of  the  trumpet-like  expansion  of  its  pharyngeiJ 
extremity  and  the  narrowness  of  its  opening  into  the  drum.  The  end  which  opens 
into  the  drum  is  formed  of  bone,  but  the  expanded  pharyngeal  end  is  formed  of  thin 
flexible  cartilage  or  gristle,  split  along  its  upper  side.  The  edges  of  the  split  are 
closed  by  a  sinewy  membrane.  By  closing  the  nose  and  mouth,  and  either  con- 
densing the  air  in  the  mouth  by  pressure,  or  rarefying  it  by  suction,  air  can  be 
respectively  driven  into  or  drawn  out  of  the  drum  through  this  tube.  At  the 
entrance  of  air  into  the  drum,  or  its  departure  &om  it,  we  feel  a  sudden  jerk  in 
the  ear,  and  bear  a  dull  crack.  Air  passes  from  the  pharynx  to  the  drum,  or  from 
the  drum  to  the  pharynx  only  at  the  moment  of  making  the  motion  of  swallowing. 

f  When  the  air  has  entered  the  drum  it  remains  there,  even  after  nose  and  mouth 
are  opened  again,  until  we  make  another  motion 
of  swallowing.  Then  the  air  leaves  the  drum, 
as  we  perceive  by  a  second  cracking  in  the  ear, 
and  the  cessation  of  the  feeling  of  tension  in  the 
drumskin  which  had  remained  up  till  that  time. 
These  experiments  shew  that  the  tube  is  not 
usually  open,  but  is  opened  only  during  swallow- 
ing, and  this  is  explained  by  the  fact  that  the 
muscles  which  raise  the  velum  paldtl  or  soft 
palate,  and  are  set  in  action  on  swallowing,  arise 
partly  from  the  cartilaginous  extremity  of  the  tube. 
Hence  the  drum  is  generally  quite  closed,  and 
filled  with  air,  which  has  a  pressure  equal  to 

m  that  of  the  external  air,  because  it  has  from 
time  to  time,  that  is  whenever  we  swallow,  the 
means  of  equalising  itself  with  the  same  by  free 
communication.  For  a  strong  pressure  of  the 
air,  the  tube  opens  even  without  the  action  of 
swallowing,  and  its  power  of  resistance  seems  to 
be  very  different  in  different  individuals. 

In  two  places,  this  air  in  the  drum  is  like- 
wise separated  from  the  fluid  of  the  labyrinth 
merely  by  a  thin  stretched  membrane,  which  closes  the  two  windows  of  the 


Mm 

Ossfdes  of  the  ear  in  xnntaal  oonnectSon. 
seen  from  the  front,  and  taken  from  the 
right  side  of  the  head,  which  has  been 
tamed  a  little  to  the  right  round  a 
rertioal  axis.  M  hammer  or  maUem, 
J  anril  or  imut.  S  stirrup  or  tiapm. 
Mcp  head,  Mc  neck,  ICl  long  prooeea  or 
procei'sus  grd'eUiiy  Mm  handle  or  fmiiiil'- 
brUan  of  the  hammer.— -Jc  body,  Jb  short 
process,  Jl  long  prooeas,  Jpl  orUcnlar 
process  or  ot  orbieOUtre  or  proett^tus  Umti- 
e^UarUf  of  the  anrll.— Sop  head  or  espir'- 
iUum  of  the  stirmp. 


*  [In  common  parlance  the  drumskin  of 
the  ear,  or  tympanic  membrafie,  is  spoken  of 
as  the  drum  itself.  Anatomists  as  well  as 
drummers  distinguish  the  membranous  cover 
(drumskin)  which  is  struck,  from  the  hollow 
cavity  (drum)  which  contains  the  resonant  air. 


The  quantities  of  the  Latin  words  are  marked, 
as  I  have  heard  musicians  give  them  incor- 
rectly.— Translator.'] 

t  [Generally    pronounced    yoo-staV-ki-an, 
but  sometimes  yoO'Stai'-shl'&n,'— Translator,', 


Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


rHAP.  VI.    SYMPATHETICALLY  VIBRATING  PARTS  OF  THE  EAR.      131 


Fio.  38. 


labyrinth,  already  mentioned,  namely,  the  oval  window  (o,  fig.  36,  p.  129c)  and 
the  round  window  (r).  Both  of  these  membranes  are  in  contact  on  their  outer 
side  with  the  air  of  the  drum,  and  on  their  inner  side  with  the  water  of  the  laby- 
rinth. The  membrane  of  the  round  window  is  free,  but  that  of  the  oval  window 
is  connected  with  the  drumskin  of  the  ear  by  a  series  of  three  Httle  bones  or 
auditory  ossicles,  jointed  together.  Fig.  37  shews  the  three  ossicles  in  their  natural 
connection,  enlarged  four  diameters.  They  are  the  hammer  (mal'leus)  M,  the  anvil 
(incus)  J,  and  the  stirrup  (sta'pes*)  S.  The  hammer  is  attached  to  the  drumskin, 
and  the  stirrup  to  the  membrane  of  the  oval  window. 

The  hammer,  shewn  separately  in  fig.  38,  has  a  thick,  rounded  upper  extremity, 
the  head  cp,  and  a  thinner  lower  extremity,  the  handle  m.     Between  these  two  is 

a  contraction  c,  the  neck.  At  the 
back  of  the  head  is  the  surface  of  the 
joint,  by  means  of  which  it  fits  on  to  ^ 
the  anvil.  Below  the  neck,  where 
the  handle  begins,  project  two  pro- 
cesses, the  long  1,  also  called  pro- 
cessus Folidnu^  and  pr,  grdcllis,  and 
the  short  b,  also  called  pr,  hre'vis. 
The  long  process  has  the  proportion- 
ate length  shewn  in  the  figure,  in 
children  only ;  in  adults  it  appears  to 
be  absorbed  down  to  a  little  stump. 

Bight  bammer  A  from  the  front.  B  from  behind     cph«id^   J^  jg  directed  forwards,  and  is  COVCrod 
c  nock,  b  short,  1  long  priKjess,  ni  handle.     •  Surface  of  i  .  i    * 

the  joint.  by  the  bands  which  fasten  the  hammer 

in  front.  The  slwrt  process  b,  on  the  other  hand,  is  directed  towards  the  drumskin, 
and  presses  its  upper  part  a  little  forwards.  From  the  point  of  this  process  b  to 
the  point  of  the  handle  m  the  hammer  is  attached  to  the  upper  portion  of  the  ^ 


Fia.  39. 


Fio.  40. 


Lrft  temporal  bone  of  a  newly-born  child,  ^ith  thfi  anditorj  Right  drumskin  with  the  hammer,  seen  from  the 

MBicles  in  tUu.    Sta,  spina  tympftnica  anterior.    Stp,  intdde.    The  inner  layer  of  the  fold  of  muooui 

xplna  tympinica  postllrior.    Mcp,  head  of  the  hammer.  membrane   belonging    to   the    hammer    (see 

Mb  ».hort.  Ml  long  process  of  hammer.     J  anvil.     S  below)    is    removed.     Btp,   spina   tympftnica 

stirmp.  post.     Mop,  head  of  the  hammer.    Ml,  long 

process  of  hammer,  ma,  ligftroen'tum  mallM 
ant.  I  chorda  tympAnl.  a  Eustachian  tube. 
*  Tendon  of  the  M.  tensor  tympftnl,  cut 
through  close  to  its  insertion. 

drumskin,  in  such  a  manner  that  the  point  of  the  handle  draws  the  drumskin 
considerably  towards  the  inner  part  of  the  ear. 

Fig.  39  above  shews  the  hammer  in  its  natural  position  as  seen  from 
without,  after  the  drumskin  has  been  removed,  and  fig.  40  shows  the  hammer 
lying  against  the  drumskin  as  seen  from  within.     The  hammer  is  fastened  along 

*  {Stapes  is  asaally  called  stdi'-p^ez.    It  is 
not  a  classical  word,  and  is  usually  received  as 


a  contraction  for  stdVipfa  or  foot-rest,  also  not 
classical.-  Transluior.] 


Digitized  by 


Google 


132     SYMPATHETICALLY  VIBBATING  PARTS  OF  THE  EAR    parti. 

the  upper  margin  of  the  drumskin  by  a  fold  of  mucous  membrane,  within  which 
run  a  series  of  rather  stiff  bundles  of  tendinous  fibres.  These  straps  arise  in  a 
line  which  passes  from  the  processus  Folianus  (fig.  38, 1),  above  the  contraction  of 
the  neck,  towards  the  lower  end  of  the  surface  of  the  joint  for  the  anvil,  and  in 
elderly  people  is  developed  into  a  prominent  ridge  of  bone.  The  tendinous  bands 
or  ligaments  are  strongest  and  stiffest  at  the  front  and  back  end  of  this  line  of 
insertion.  The  front  portion  of  the  ligament,  lig.  mallei  anterius  (fig.  40,  ma), 
surrounds  the  processus  Folianus,  and  is  attached  partly  to  a  bony  spine  (figs.  39 
and  40,  Stp)  of  the  osseous  ring  of  the  drum,  which  projects  close  to  the  neck  of 
the  hammer,  and  partly  to  its  under  edge,  and  partly  falls  into  a  bony  fissure 
which  leads  towards  the  articulation  of  the  jaw.  The  back  portion  of  the  same 
hgament,  on  the  other  hand,  is  attached  to  a  sharp-edged  bony  ridge  projecting 
inwards  firom  the  drumskin,  and  parallel  to  it,  a  little  above  the  opening,  through 

f  which  a  traversing  nerve,  the  chorda  tymp&nl  (fig.  40, 1, 1,  p.  131c),  enters  the  bone. 
This  second  bundle  of  fibres  may  be  called  the  lig.  mall§!  posterius.  In  fig.  39 
(p.  1310)  the  origin  of  this  ligament  is  seen  as  a  httle  projection  of  the  ring  to 
which  the  drumskin  is  attached.  This  projection  bounds  towards  the  right  the 
upper  edge  of  the  opening  for  the  drumskin,  which  begins  to  the  left  of  Stp,  exactly 
at  the  place  where  the  long  process  of  the  anvil  makes  its  appearance  in  the  figure. 
These  two  ligaments,  front  and  back,  taken  together  form  a  moderately  tense 
sinewy  chord,  round  which  the  hammer  can  turn  as  on  an  axis.  Hence  even  when 
the  two  other  ossicles  have  been  carefcdly  removed,  without  loosening  these  two 
Hgaments,  the  hammer  will  remain  in  its  natural  position,  although  not  so  stiffly 
as  before. 

The  middle  fibres  of  the  broad  ligamentous  band  above  mentioned  pass  outwards 
towards  the  upper  bony  edge  of  the  drumskin.  They  are  comparatively  short,  and 
are  known  as  lig.  maJlei  externum.    Arising  above  the  line  of  the  axis  of  the 

^  hammer,  they  prevent  the  head  from  turning  too  far  inwards,  and  the  handle  with 
the  drumskin  from  turning  too  far  outwards,  and  oppose  any  down-dragging  of  the 
ligament  forming  the  axis.  The  first  effect  is  increased  by  a  ligament  (lig.  mallei 
superius)  which  passes  from  the  processus  Folianus,  upwards,  into  the  small  sht, 
between  the  head  of  the  hammer  and  the  wall  of  the  drum,  as  shewn  in  fig,  40 
(p.  131c). 

It  must  be  observed  that  in  the  upper  part  of  the  channel  of  the  Eustachian 
tube,  there  is  a  miiscUfor  tightening  the  drumskin  (m.  tensor  tymp&nl),  the  tendon 
of  which  passes  obliquely  across  the  cavity  of  the  drum  and  is  attached  to  the 
upper  part  of  the  handle  of  the  hammer  (at*,  fig.  40,  p.  131c).  This  muscle 
must  be  regarded  as  a  moderately  tense  elastic  band,  and  may  have  its  tension 
temporarily  much  increased  by  active  contraction.  The  effect  of  this  muscle  is 
also  principally  to  draw  the  handle  of  the  hammer  inwards,  together  with  the 
drumskin.    But  since  its  point  of  attachment  is  so  close  to  the  ligamentous  axis, 

^  the  chief  part  of  its  pull  acts  on  this  axis,  stretching  it  as  it  draws  it  inwards. 
Here  we  must  observe  that  in  the  case  of  a  rectilinear  inextensible  cord,  which 
is  moderately  tense,  such  as  the  ligamentous  axis  of  the  hammer,  a  slight  force 
which  pulls  it  sideways,  suffices  to  produce  a  very  considerable  increase  of  tension. 
This  is  the  case  with  the  present  arrangement  of  stretching  muscles.  It  should 
also  be  remembered  that  quiescent  muscles  not  excited  by  innervation,  are  always 
stretched  elastically  in  the  living  body,  and  act  like  elastic  bands.  This  elastic 
tension  can  of  course  be  considerably  increased  by  the  innervation  which  brings 
the  muscles  into  action,  but  such  tension  is  never  entirely  absent  from  the  majority 
of  oiu"  muscles. 

The  anvil,  which  is  shown  separately  in  fig.  41,  resembles  a  double  tooth  with 
two  fangs ;  the  surface  of  its  joint  with  the  hammer  (at  *,  fig.  41),  replacing  the 
masticating  surface.  Of  the  two  roots  of  the  tooth  which  are  rather  widely 
separated,  the  upper,  directed  backwards,  is  called  the  short  process  b ;  the  other, 
thinner  and  directed  downwards,  the  long  process  of  the  anvil  1.    At  the  tip  of 

Digitized  by  V^OOQIC 


CHAP.  VI.  SYMPATHETICALLY  VIBRATING  PARTS  OF  THE  EAR.   133 

the  latter  is  the  knob  which  articulates  with  the  stirrup.  The  tip  of  the  short 
process,  on  the  other  hand,  by  means  of  a  short  ligament  and  an  imperfectly 
yj^    ^  g  developed  joint  at  its  under  surface,  is  con- 

nected with  the  back  wall  of  the  cavity  of 
the  drum,  at  the  spot  where  this  passes 
backwards  into  the  air  cavities  of  the  mastoid 
process  behind  the  ear.  The  joint  between 
anvil  and  hammer  is  a  curved  depression  of 
a  rather  irregular  form,  like  a  saddle.  In 
its  action  it  maybe  compared  with  the  joints 
of  the  well-known  Breguet  watchkeys,  which 
have  rows  of  interlocking  teeth,  offering 
scarcely  any  resistance  to  revolution  in  one 
Bight  BXkru.  A  medial  surface.  B  front  yiew.  o    direction,  but  allowing  no  rcvolution  what-  ^ 

body,   b  short,  1  long  process,   pi  proc.  lenticu-  ^«^y    :„    +1^0    nfhfir        TntArlnokinff    tfipth  of 

lAris  or  os  orbiculftre.    •  Articulation  with  the  ®^®^    "^    '"^    OlOer.        inieriOCKmg    TOCin   OI 

head  of  the  hammer.  •*  Surface  resting  on  the  tluS  kind  are  dcveloped  UPOU  the  Under  side 

wall  of  the  drum.  i?xT-''xi-i.i.  j  -i 

of  the  jomt  between  hammer  and  anvil. 
The  tooth  on  the  hammer  projects  towards  the  drumskin,  that  of  the  anvil  lies 
inwards ;  and,  conversely,  towards  the  upper  end  of  the  hollow  of  the  joint,  the 
anvil  projects  outwards,  and  the  hammer  inwards.  The  consequence  of  this 
arrangement  is  that  when  the  hammer  is  drawn  inwards  by  the  handle,  it  bites 
the  anvil  firmly  and  carries  it  with  it.  Conversely,  when  the  drumskin,  with  the 
hammer,  is  driven  outwards,  the  anvil  is  not  obliged  to  foUow  it.  The  interlocking 
teeth  of  the  surfaces  of  the  joint  then  separate,  and  the  surfaces  glide  over  each 
other  with  very  Uttle  friction.  This  arrangement  has  the  very  great  advantage  of 
preventing  any  possibility  of  the  stirrup's  being  torn  away  from  the  oval  window, 
when  the  air  in  the  auditory  passage  is  considerably  rarefied.  There  is  also  no 
danger  from  driving  in  the  hammer,  as  might  happen  when  the  air  in  the  auditory  ^ 
passage  was  condensed,  because  it  is  powerfully  opposed  by  the  tension  of  the 
drumskin,  which  is  drawn  in  like  a  funnel. 

When  air  is  forced  into  the  cavity  of  the  drum  in  the  act  of  swallowing,  the 
contact  of  hammer  and  anvil  is  loosened.  Weak  tones  in  the  middle  and  upper 
regions  of  the  scale  are  then  not  heard  much  more  weakly  than  usual,  but  stronger 
tones  are  very  sensibly  damped.  This  may  perhaps  be  explained  by  supposing  that 
the  adhesion  of  the  articulating  surfaces  suffices  to  transfer  weak  motions  from  one 
bone  to  the  other,  but  that  strong  impulses  cause  the  surfaces  to  sHde  over  one 
another,  and  hence  the  tones  due  to  such  impulses  must  be  enfeebled. 

Deep  tones  are  damped  in  this  case,  whether  they  are  strong  or  weak,  perhaps 
because  these  always  require  larger  motions  to  become  audible.* 

Another  important  effect  on  the  apprehension  of  tone,  which  is  due  to  the  above 
arrangement  in  the  articulation  of  hammer  and  anvil,  will  have  to  be  considered  in 
relation  to  combinational  tones.    [See  p.  158&.]  ^ 

Since  the  attachment  of  the  tip  of  the  short  process  of  the  anvil  lies  sensibly 
inwards  and  above  the  ligamentous  axis  of  the  hammer,  the  head  of  the  hammer 
separates  from  the  articulating  surface  between  hammer  and  anvil,  when  the  head 
is  driven  outwards,  and  therefore  the  handle  and  drumskin  are  driven  inwards. 
The  consequence  is  that  the  ligaments  holding  the  anvil  against  the  hammer,  and 
on  the  tip  of  the  short  process  of  the  anvil,  are  sensibly  stretched,  and  hence  the 
tip  is  raised  from  its  osseous  support.  Consequently  in  the  normal  position  of  the 
ossicles  for  hearing,  the  anvil  has  no  contact  with  any  other  bone  but  the  hammer, 
and  both  bones  are  held  in  position  only  by  stretched  ligaments,  which  are  tolerably 
tight,  so  that  only  the  revolution  of  the  hammer  about  its  ligamentous  axis  remains 
comparatively  free. 

The  third  ossicle,  the  stirrup,  shewn  separately  in  fig.  42,  has  really  a  most 
striking  resemblance  to  the  implement  after  which  it  has  been  named.    The  foot  B 
*  On  this  point  see  Part  II.  Chapter  IX. 

Digitized  by  V^OOQIC 


134     SYMPATHETICALLY  VIBKATING  PARTS  OF  THE   EAIl.    parti. 

is  fastened  into  the  membrane  of  the  oval  window,  and  fills  it  all  up,  with  the 
exception  of  a  narrow  margin.  The  head  op,  has  an  articulating  hole  for  the  tip 
of  the  long  process  of  the  anvil 

(processus    lenticol&ris,    or    os         ^  ^  ^^-  ^'-  ^ 

orbiculare).     The  joint  is  sur- 
rounded by   a    lax  membrane.  f^^^^\ 
When  the  drumskin  is  normally  ^^^"^ 
drawn  inwards,  the  anvil  presses  ^^ 

on  the  stirrup,  so  that  no  tighter  _£_  p 

ligamentous    fiebstening    of    the  ^ 

loint    ifl    TififtAB«ii.TV         l?.v«rv    in      Right  stirrup :  seen, -4  from  within,  5  from  front,  C from  behind. ' 
JOmi    IS    necessary.       J^jVery    m-  ^iq^^^    cp,  head  or  capitulum.    a  Front,  p  back  llmb. 

crease  in  the  push  on  the  hammer 

arising  from  the  drumskin  also  occasions  an  increiftse  in  the  push  of  the  stirrup 
^  against  the  oval  window  ;  but  in  this  action  the  upper  and  somewhat  looser 
margin  of  its  foot  is  more  displaced  than  the  under,  so  that  the  head  rises  slightly ; 
this  motion  again  causes  a  slight  elevation  of  the  tip  of  the  long  process  in  the 
anvil,  in  the  direction  conditioned  by  its  position,  inwards  and  underneath  the 
ligamentous  axis  of  the  hammer. 

The  excursions  of  the  foot  of  the  stirrup  are  always  very  small,  and  according 
to  my  measurements*  never  exceed  one-tenth  of  a  millimetre  ('00394  or  about 
^^^  of  an  inch).  But  the  hammer  when  freed  fr'om  anvil  and  stirrup,  with  its 
handle  moving  outwards,  and  sliding  over  the  atticulating  surface  of  the  anvil,  can 
make  excursions  at  least  nine  times  as  great  as  it  can  execute  when  acting  in 
connection  with  anvil  and  stirrup. 

The  first  advantage  of  the  apparatus  belonging  to  the  drum  of  the  ear,  is  tliat 
the  whole  sonorous  motion  of  the  comparatively  wide  surface  of  the  drumskin  (ver- 
tical diameter  9  to  10  millimetres,  [or  0*35  to  0*39  inches,]  just  over  one- third  of  an 
^  inch ;  horizontal  diameter,  7*5  to  9  millimetres,  [or  0*295  ^  ^'35  ii^ches,]  that  is 
about  five-sixths  of  the  former  dimensions)  is  collected  and  transferred  by  the 
ossicles  to  the  relatively  much  smaller  surface  of  the  oval  window  or  of  the  foot  of 
the  stirrup,  which  is  only  1*5  to  3  millimetres  [o*o6  to  0*12  inches]  in  diameter. 
The  surface  of  the  drumskin  is  hence  15  to  20  times  larger  than  that  of  the  oval 
window. 

Li  this  transference  of  the  vibrations  of  air  into  the  labyrinth  it  is  to  be  observed 
that  though  the  particles  of  air  themselves  have  a  comparatively  large  amplitude  of 
vibration-,  yet  their  density  is  so  small  that  they  have  no  very  great  moment  of  inertia, 
and  consequently  when  their  motion  is  impeded  by  the  drumskin  of  the  ear,  they 
are  not  capable  of  presenting  much  resistance  to  such  an  impediment,  or  of  exert- 
ing any  sensible  pressure  against  it.  The  fluid  in  the  labyrinth,  on  the  other  hand, 
is  much  denser  and  heavier  than  the  air  in  the  auditory  passage,  and  for  moving  it 
rapidly  backwards  and  forwards  as  in  sonorous  oscillations,  a  feir  greater  exertion  of 
^  pressure  is  required  than  was  necessary  for  the  air  in  the  auditory  passage.  On 
the  other  hand  the  amplitude  of  the  vibrations  performed  by  the  fluid  in  the  laby- 
rinth are  relatively  very  small,  and  extremely  minute  vibrations  will  in  this  case 
suffice  to  give  a  vibratory  motion  to  the  terminations  and  appendages  of  the  nerves, 
which  lie  on  the  very  limits  of  microscopic  vision. 

The  mechanical  problem  which  the  apparatus  within  the  drum  of  the  ear  had 
to  solve,  was  to  transform  a  motion  of  great  amplitude  and  little  force,  such  as  im- 
pinges on  the  drumskin,  into  a  motion  of  small  amplitude  and  great  force,  such  as 
had  to  be  conimunicated  to  the  fluid  in  the  labyrinth. 

A  problem  of  this  sort  can  be  solved  by  various  kinds  of  mechanical  apparatus, 
such  as  levers,  trains  of  pulleys,  cranes,  and  the  like.  The  mode  in  which  it  is 
solved  by  the  apparatus  in  the  drum  of  the  ear,  is  quite  unusual,  and  very  peculiar. 

*  Helmholtz,  *  Mechanism  of  the  Auditory  attempt  is  made  to  prove  the  correctness  of 
Ossicles/  in  Pfiucger's  Archiv  fUr  Physio-  the  account  of  this  mechanism  given  in  the 
hijie    vol.  i.  pp.   34  43.     In  this   paper   an      text. 

Digitized  by  V^OOQIC 


CHAP.  VI.   SYMPATHETICALLY  VIBRATING  PARTS  OF  THE   EAR.    135 

A  leverage  is  certainly  employed,  but  only  to  a  moderate  extent.  The  tip  of 
the  handle  of  the  hammet,  on  which  the  poll  of  the  drumskin  first  acts»  is  about 
once  and  a  half  as  far  from  the  axis  of  rotation  as  that  point  of  the  anvil  which 
presses  on  the  stirrup  (see  fig.  39,  p.  1 31c).  The  handle  of  the  hammer  consequently 
forms  the  longer  arm  of  a  lever,  and  the  pressure  on  the  stirrup  will  be  once  and  a 
half  as  great  as  that  which  drives  in  the  hammer. 

The  chief  means  of  reinforcement  is  due  to  the  form  of  the  drumskin.  It  has 
been  already  mentioned  that  its  middle  or  navel  (umbilicus)  is  drawn  inwards  by 
the  handle,  so  as  to  present  a  funnel  shape.  But  the  meridian  lines  of  this  funnel 
drawn  from  the  navel  to  the  circumference,  are  not  straight  lines  ;  they  are  sHghtiy 
convex  on  the  outer  side.  A  diminution  of  pressure  in  the  auditory  passage  in- 
creases this  convexity,  and  an  augmentation  diminishes  it.  Now  the  tension  caused 
in  an  inextensible  thread,  having  the  form  of  a  flat  arch,  by  a  force  acting  perpen- 
dicular to  its  convexity,  is  very  considerable.  It  is  well  known  that  a  sensible  force  ^ 
must  be  exerted  to  stretch  a  long  thin  string  into  even  a  tolerably  straight  horizon- 
tal line.  The  force  is  indeed  very  much  greater  than  the  weight  of  the  string  which 
pulls  the  string  from  the  horizontal  position.*  In  the  case  of  the  drumskin,  it  is 
not  gravity  which  prevents  its  radial  fibres  from  straightening  themselves,  but  partly 
the  pressure  of  the  air",  and  partly  the  elastic  pull  of  the  circular  fibres  of  the  mem- 
brane. The  latter  tend  to  contract  towards  the  axis  of  the  funnel-shaped  mem- 
brane, and  hence  produce  the  inflection  of  the  radial  fibres  towards  this  axis.  By 
means  of  the  variable  pressure  of  air  during  the  sonorous  vibrations  of  the  at- 
mosphere this  pull  exerted  by  the  circular  fibres  is  alternately  strengthened  and 
weakened,  and  produces  an  effect  on  the  point  where  the  radial  fibres  are  attached 
to  the  tip  of  the  handle  of  the  hammer,  similar  to  that  which  would  happen  if  we 
could  alternately  increase  and  diminish  the  weight  of  a  string  stretched  horizontally, 
for  this  would  produce  a  proportionate  increase  and  decrease  in  the  pull  exerted  by 
the  hand  which  stretched  it.  ^ 

In  a  horizontally  stretched  string  such  as  has  been  just  described,  it  should  be 
further  remarked  that  an  extremely  small  relaxation  of  the  hand  is  followed  by  a 
considerable  fall  in  the  middle  of  the  string.  The  relaxation  of  the  hand,  namely, 
takes  place  in  the  direction  of  the  chord  of  the  arc,  and  easy  geometrical  con- 
siderations shew  that  chords  of  arcs  of  the  same  length  and  different,  but  always 
very  small  curvature,  differ  very  slightly  indeed  from  each  other  and  from  the 
lengths  of  the  arcs  themselves.f  This  is  also  the  case  with  the  drumskin.  An  ex- 
tremely little  yielding  in  the  handle  of  the  hammer  admits  of  a  very  considerable 
change  in  the  curvature  of  the  drumskin.  The  consequence  is  that,  in  sonorous 
vibrations,  the  parts  of  the  drumskin  which  lie  between  the  inner  attachment  of 
this  membrane  to  the  hammer  and  its  outer  attachment  to  the  ring  of  the  drum, 
are  able  to  follow  the  oscillations  of  the  air  with  considerable  freedom,  while  the 
motion  of  the  air  is  transmitted  to  the  handle  of  the  hammer  with  much  diminished 
amplitude  but  much  increased  force.  After  this,  as  the  motion  passes  from  the  ^ 
handle  of  the  hammer  to  the  stirrup,  the  leverage  already  mentioned  causes 
a  second  and  more  moderate  reduction  of  the  amplitude  of  vibration  with  corre- 
sponding increase  of  force. 

We  now  proceed  to  describe  the  innermost  division  of  thes)rgan  of  hearing* 
called  the  labyrinth.  Fig.  43  (p.  134c)  represents  a  cast  of  its  cavity,  as  seen  from 
different  positions.  Its  middle  portion,  containing  the  oval  window  Fv  (fenestra 
vestibull)  that  receives  the  foot  of  the  stirrup,  is  called  the  vestibule  of  the  labyrinth. 

*  [The  following  qaatrain,  said  to  have  Into  a  horizontal  line, 

been  unconsciously  produced  by  Vince,  as  a  So  as  to  make  it  truly  straight. — TratuUUor.] 

coroUary^  to  one  of  the  propositions  in  his  .  rj^e  amount  of  difference  varies  as  the 

•  Mechamcs/  will  serve  to  impress  the  fact  1^  ^^  ^^e  depth  of  the  arc.    If  the  length 


on  a  non-mathematical  nader :—  ^^  ^j^^  ^  ^^  j  ^^j  t^,,  distance  of  its  middle 

Hence  no  force,  however  great,  from  the  chord  be  «,  tl 

Can  stretch  a  cord,  however  fine,  the  arc  by  the  length 


3      2 
Digitized  by  VjOOQIC 


136 


LABYRINTH  OF  THE   EAR. 


PAKT  I. 


From  the  vestibule  proceeds  forwards  and  underwards,  a  spiral  caiial,  the  snail- 
shell  or  cochlea,  at  the  entrance  to  which  hes  the  rouTid  window  Fc  (fenestra 
cochleae),  which  is  turned  towards  the  cavity  of  the  drum.  Upwards  and  back- 
wards, on  the  other  hand,  proceed  three  semicircular  canals  from  the  vestibule,  the 
horizontal,  front  vertical ^  and  back  vertical  semicircular  canals,  each  of  which 
debouches  with  both  its  mouths  in  the  vestibule,  and  each  of  which  has  at  one 
end  a  bottle- shaped  enlargement,  or  ampulla  (ha,  vaa,  vpa).  The  aquaeductus 
vestlbuli  shewn  in  the  figure,  Av,  appears  (from  Dr.  Fr.  E.  Weber's  investigations) 
to  form  a  communication  between  tlie  water  of  the  labyrinth,  and  the  spaces  for 
lymph  within  the  cranium.  The  rough  places  Tsf  and  •  are  casts  of  canals  which 
introduce  nerves. 

The  whole  of  this  cavity  of  the  labyrinth  is  filled  with  fluid,  and  surrounded  by 
the  extremely  hard  close  mass  of  the  petrous  bone,  so  that  there  are  only  two 
'^  yielding  spots  on  its  walls,  the  two  windows,  the  oval  Fv,  and  the  round  Fc.  Into 
the  first,  as  already  described,  is  fastened  the  foot  of  the  stirrup,  by  a  narrow 
membranous  margin.  The  second  is  closed  by  a  membrane.  When  the  stirrup 
is  driven  against  the  oval  window,  the  whole  mass  of  fluid  in  the  labyrinth  is 
necessarily  driven  against  the  round  window,  as  the  only  spot  where  its  walls  can 
give  way.  If,  as  Politzer  did,  we  put  a  finely  drawn  glass  tube  as  a  manometer 
into  the  romid  window,  without  in  other  respects  injuring^he  labyrinth,  the  water 
in  this  tube  will  be  driven  upwards  as  soon  as  ar^strong  pressure  of  air  acts  on  the 

Fig.  43. 


Rp  vaa 


A 

\ 

1..  ^ 

V^ 

F  ^ 

^ 

r-- 

Tpa 


A,  left  labyrinth  from  without.  B,  right  labyrinth  from  within.  C,  left  labyrinth  from  abore.  Fc,  fenestra 
cochleae  or  rrjuud  window.  Pr,  fenestra  TestTbail,  or  oval  window.  Re,  recessns  elliptTcus.  Rs,  recessus 
sphaerlcus.  h,  horizontal  t<euiicircnlar  canal,  ha,  ampulla  of  the  same,  raa,  ampulla  of  the  front  rertical 
semicircular  canal,  ypa,  ampulla  of  the  back  Tertical  semicircular  canal,  tc,  common  limb  of  the  two  rertioal 
semicircular  canals.  Ar,  cast  of  the  aquaeductus  restlbfill.  Tgf,  traotus  splrftlis  forftmlnteus.  *  Cast  of  the 
little  canals  which  debouch  on  the  pj^rftmis  vestlbtUI. 

outside  of  the  drumskin  and  causes  the  foot  of  the  stirrup  to  be  driven  into  the  oval 
window. ; 

The  terminations  of  the  auditory  nerve  are  spread  over  fine  membranous 
^  formations,  which  lie  partly  floating  and  partly  expanded  in  the  hollow  of  the  bony 
labyrinth,  and  taken  together  compose  the  membranous  labyrinth.  This  last  has 
on  the  whole  the  same  shape  as  the  bony  labyrinth.  But  its  canals  and  ca\4ties 
are  smaller,  and  its  interior  is  divided  into  two  separate  sections;  first  the 
utricUlus  with  the  semicircular  canals,  and  second  the  saccUlus  with  the  mem- 
branous  cochin.  Both  the  utriculus  and  the  sacculus  lie  in  the  vestibule  of  the 
bony  labyrinth  ;  the  utriculus  opposite  to  the  recessus  ellipttcus  (Ke,  fig.  43  above), 
the  sacculus  opposite  to  the  recessus  sphaericus  (Rs).  These  are  floating  bags 
filled  with  water,  and  only  touch  the  wall  of  the  labyrinth  at  the  point  where  the 
nerves  enter  them. 

The  form  of  the  utriculus  with  its  membranous  semicircular  canals  is  shewn  in 
fig.  44.  The  ampullae  project  much  more  in  the  membranous  than  in  the  bony  semi- 
circular canals.  According  to  the  recent  investigations  of  Riidinger,  the  mem- 
branous semicircular  canals  do  not  float  in  the  bony  ones,  but  are  fastened  to  tlie 
convex  side  of  the  latter.     lu  each  ampulla  there  is  a  pad -like  prominence  directed 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


CH.4P.  VI. 


COCHLEA  OF  THE  EAR. 


137 


inwards,  into  which  fibriles  of  the  auditory  nerve  enter ;  and  on  the  utriculus 
there  is  a  place  which  is  flatter  and  thickened.    The  peculiar  manner  in  which 

the  nerves  terminate  in  this  place  will  be  described 
hereafter.  Whether  these,  and  the  whole  apparatus 
of  the  semicircular  canals,  assist  in  the  sensation  of 
hearing,  has  latterly  been  rendered  very  doubtful.    [See 

p.  151^-] 

In  the  inside  of  the  utriculus  is  found  the  auditory 
sand,  consisting  of  little  crystals  of  lime  connected  by 
means  of  a  mucous  mass  with  each  other  and  with  the 
thickened  places  where  the  nerves  are  so  abundant. 
In  the  hollow  of  the  bony  vestibule,  near  the  utriculus, 
and  fastened  to  it,  but  not  communicating  with  it,  lies 
the  sacculus,  also  provided  with  a  similar  thickened^ 
utricaina  and  inembmiions  aemicir-  gp^^  f^n  Qf  nervos.    A  narrow  caual  counccts  it  with 

cQur  canals  (left  side)  seen  from    *^ 

witboQt.  TH  front,  rp  back  rertioai,  the  caual  of  the  membrauous  cochlea.    As  to  the  cavity 

h  horizontal  semicircular  canal.  •   ,,  1  «  •■        />  'x       n     j    '^    • 

of  the  cochlea,  we  see  by  ng.  43  opposite,  that  it  is 
exactly  similar  to  the  shell  of  a  garden  snail ;  but  the  canal  of  the  cochlea  is 
divided  into  two  almost  completely  separated  galleries,  by  a  transverse  partition, 
partly  bony  and  partly  membranous.  These  galleries  communicate  only  at  the 
vertex  of  the  cochlea  through  a  small  opening,  the  hSlicotrema,  bounded  by  the 
hamulus  or  hook-shaped  termination  of  its  central  axis  or  viddl'dltis.  Of  the  two 
galleries  into  which  the  cavity  of  the  bony  cochlea  is  divided,  one  communicates 
directly  with  the  vestibule  and  is  hence  called  the  vestibule  gallery  (scala  vestibuli). 
The  other  gallery  is  cut  off  from  the  vestibule  by  the  membranous  partition,  but 
jtist  at  its  base,  where  it  begins,  is  the  round  window,  and  the  yielding  membrane, 
which  closes  this,  allows  the  fluid  in  the  gallery  to  exchange  vibrations  with  the 
air  in  the  drum.    Hence  this  is  called  the  drum  gallery  (scala  tymp&ni).  5! 

Finally,  it  must  be  observed  that  the  membranous  partition  of  the  cochlea  is 
not  a  single  membrane,  but  a  membranous  canal  (ductus  cochlearis).     Its  inner 

margin  is  turned  towards  the  central  axis  or 
m6di6lus,  and  attached  to  the  rudimentary 
bony  partition  (l&mina  spirahs).  A  part  of 
the  opposite  external  surface  is  attached  to  the 
inner  surface  of  the  bony  gallery.  Fig.  45 
shews  the  bony  parts  of  a  cochlea  which  has 
been  laid  open,  and  flg.  46  (p.  138a),  a  trans- 
verse section  of  the  canal  (which  is  imperfect 
on  the  left  hand  at  bottom).  In  both  figures 
Ls  denotes  the  bony  part  of  the  parUtion,  and 
in  fig.  46  V  and  b  are  the  two  unattached  parts 
of  the  membranous  canal.  The  transverse  ^ 
section  of  this  canal  is,  as  the  figure  shews, 
nearly  triangular,  so  that  an  angle  of  the 
triangle  near  Lis  is  attached  to  the  edge  of 
the  bony  partition.  The  commencement  of 
the  ductus  cochlearis  at  the  base  of  tlie 
cochlea,  communicates,  as  already  stated,  by 
means  of  a  narrow  membranous  canal  with 
the  sacculus  in  the  vestibule.  Of  the  two  un- 
attached strips  of  its  membranous  walls,  the 
one  facing  the  vestibule  gallery  is  a  soft  mem- 
brane, incapable  of  offering  much  resistance — Beissner's  membrane  (membrana  vesti- 
bularis, V,  fig.  46,  p.  138a) ;  but  the  other,  the  membrana  bdslldris  (b),  is  a  firm, 
tightly  stretched,  elastic  membrane,  striped  radially,  corresponding  to  its  radial 
fibres.    It  splits  easily  in  the  direction  of  these  fibres,  shewing  that  it  is  but  loosely 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQ IC 


Pig.  45- 


Bony  cochlea  (right  side)  opened  in  front.  Md, 
uodlSIud.  Ls,  lAmTna  splr&Iis.  H,  h&nin  u.4. 
?ec,  fenestra  cochleae,  t  Section  of  the  partition 
of  the  cochlea,    tt  Upper  extremity  of  the  Mime. 


138 


COCHLEA  OF  THE  EAR. 


FABT  I, 


connected  in  a  direction  transverse  to  them.  The  terminations  of  the  nerves  of 
the  cochlea  and  their  appendages,  are  attached  to  the  membrana  b&silaris,  as  is 
shewn  by  the  dotted  hnes  in  fig.  46. 

When  the  drumskin  is  driven  inwards  by  increased  pressure  of  air  in  the  auditory 
passage,  it  also  forces  the  auditory  ossicles  inwards,  as  already  explained,  and  as  a 
consequence  the  foot  of  the  stiiTup  ^ 


Transyerse  section  of  a  spire  of  a  cochlea  which  has  been 
softened  in  hydrochloric  acid.  Ls,  lAmTna  spIHUis.  Lis, 
limbos  Iftmlnae  splr&lis.  Sr,  sc&la  TestlbOlI.  St,  scala 
tAmpftnl.  Dc,  ductus  oochleArls.  Lsp,  ITgftmentum 
splr&le.  T,  membrflna  TentibulAris.  b,  membrftna  b&sUAris. 
e,  outer  wall  of  the  ductus  oochleftrls.  *  its  fillet.  The 
dotted  lines  shew  sections  of  the  membrftna  tectAria  and 
the  auditory  rods. 


penetrates  deeper  into  the  oval  window 

The  fluid  of  the  labyrinth,  being  sur- 
rounded in  all  other  places  by  firm 

bony  walls,  has  only  one  means  of 

escape, — the  round  window  with  its 

yielding  membrane.     To  reach  it,  the 

fluid  of  the  labyrinth  must  either  pass 
%  through  the  hSlicdtrema,  the  narrow 

opening  at  the  vertex  of  the  cochlea, 

flowing  over  from  the  vestibule  gallery 

into  the  drum  gallery,  or,  as  it  would 

probably  not  have  sufficient  time  to  do 

this  in  the  case  of  sonorous  vibrations, 

press  the  membranous  partition  of  the 

cochlea  against  the  drum  gallery.    The 

converse  action  must  take  place  when 

the  air  in  the  auditory  passage  is  rare- 
fied. 

Hence  the  sonorous  vibrations  of  the  air  in  the  outer  auditory  passage  are 

finally  transferred  to  the  membranes  of  the  labyrinth,  more  especially  those  of  the 

cochlea,  and  to  the  expansions  of  the  nerves  upon  them. 
51        The  terminal  expansions  of  these  nerves,  as  I  have  already  mentioned,  are  con- 
nected with  very  small  elastic  appendages,  which  appear  adapted  to  excite  the 

nerves  by  their  vibrations. 

The  nerves  of  the  vestibule  terminate  in  the  thickened  places  of  the  bags  of 

the  membranous  labyrinth,  already  mentioned 

(p.  137a),  where  the  tissue  has  a  greater  and 

almost  cartilaginous  consistency.     One  of  these 

places  provided  with  nerves,  projects  hke  a  fillet 

into  the  inner  part  of  the  ampulla  of  each  semi- 
circular canal,  and  another  lies  on  each  of  the  little 

bags  in  the  vestibule.    The  nerve  fibres  here  enter 

between  the  soft  cylindrical  cells  of  the  fine  cuticle 

(Spithglium)  which  covers  the  internal  surface  of 

the  fillets.    Projecting  from  the  internal  surface 
^  of  this  epithelium  in  the  ampullae.  Max  Schultze 

discovered  a  number  of  very  peculiar,  stiff,  elastic 

hairs,  shewn  in  fig.  47.     They  are  much  longer 

than  the  vibratory  hairs  of  the  ciliated  epithelium 

(their  length  is  ^y  of  a  Paris  line,  [or  '00355 

English  inch,]  in  the  ray  fish),  brittle,  and  running 

to  a  very  fine  point.    It  is  clear  that  fine  stiff 

hairs  of  this  kind  are  extremely  well  adapted  for 

moving  sympathetically  with  the  motion  of  the 

fluid,  and  hence  for  producing  mechanical  irri- 
tation in  the  nerve  fibres  which  He  in  the  soft 

epithelium  between  their  roots. 

According  to  Max  Schultze,  the  corresponding 

thickened  fillets  in  the  vestibules,  where  the  nerves  tenninate,  have  a  similar  soft 

epithelium,  and  have  short  hairs  which  are  easily  destroyed.     Close  to  these 

Digitized  by  VjOOQlC 


Pio.  47. 


CHAP.  VI. 


COCHLEA  OP  THE  EAR. 


139 


¥1G.  48. 


surfjAces,  which  are  covered  with  nerves,  lie  the  calcareous  concretions,  called 
auditory  stones  (6t6lith8),  which  in  fishes  form  connected  convexo-concave  solids, 
aliewing  on  their  convex  side  an  impression  of  the  nerve  fillet.  In  human  heings, 
on  the  other  hand,  the  otoliths  are  heaps  of  little  crystalline  bodies,  of  a  longish 
angular  form,  lying  close  to  the  membrane  of  the  little  bags,  and  apparently 
attached  to  it.  These  otoUths  seem  also  extremely  well  suited  for  producing  a 
mechanical  irritation  of  the  nerves  whenever  the  fluid  in  the  labyrinth  is  suddenly 
agitated.  The  fine  light  membrane,  with  its  interwoven  nerves,  probably  instantly 
follows  the  motion  of  the  fluid,  whereas  the  heavier  crystals  are  set  more  slowly  in 
motion,  and  hence  also  yield  up  their  motion  more  slowly,  and  thus  partly  drag 
and  partly  squeeze  the  adjacent  nerves.  This  would  satisfy  the  same  conditions 
of  exciting  nerves,  as  Heidenhain^s  tetdnomdtor.  By  this  instrument  the  nerve 
which  acts  on  a  muscle  is  exposed  to  the  action  of  a  very  rapidly  oscillating  ivory 
hanuner,  which  at  every  blow  squeezes  without  bruising  the  nerve.  A  powerful 
and  continuous  excitement  of  the  nerve  is  thus  produced,  which  is  shewn  by  a  ^ 
powerful  and  continuous  contraction  of  the  corresponding  muscle.  The  above  parts 
of  the  ear  seem  to  be  well  suited  to  produce  similar  mechanical  excitement. 

The  construction  of  the  cochlea  is  much  more  complex.  The  nerve  fibres  enter 
through  the  axis  or  modiolus  of  the  cochlea  into  the  bony  part  of  the  partition, 
and  then  come  on  to  the  membranous  part.  Where  they  reach  this,  peculiar 
formations  were  discovered  quite  recently  (1851)  by  the  Marchese  Corti,  and  have 
been  named  after  him.     On  these  the  nerves  terminate. 

The  expansion  of  the  cochlean  nerve  is  shewn  in  fig.  48.  It  enters  through 
the  axis  (2)  and  sends  out  its  fibres  in  a  radial  direction  from  the  axis  through  the 

bony  partition  (1,3,  4), 
as  far  as  its  margins. 
At  this  point  the  nerves 
pass  under  the  com- 
mencement of  the  mem-  % 
brana  basilaris,  pene- 
trate this  in  a  series  of 
openings,  and  thus  reach 
the  ductus  cochlearis 
and  those  nervous, 
elastic  formations  which 
lie  on  the  inner  zone 
(Zi)  of  the  membrane. 

The  margin  of  the 
bony  partition  (a  to  b, 
flg*  49*  P*  140a),  and  the 
inner  zone  of  the  mem- 
brana  basilaris  (a  a')  are  shewn  after  Hensen.  The  under  side  of  the  figure  * 
corresponds  with  the  scala  tymp&ui,  the  upper  with  the  ductus  cochlearis.  Here  h  ^ 
at  the  top  and  k  at  the  bottom,  are  the  two  plates  of  the  bony  partition,  between 
which  the  expansion  of  the  nerve  b  proceeds.  The  upper  side  of  the  bony  parti- 
tion bears  a  fillet  of  close  hgamentous  tissue  (Z,  fig.  49,  also  shewn  at  Lis,  fig.  46, 
p.  138a),  which,  on  account  of  the  toothlike  impressions  on  its  upper  side,  is  called 
the  toothed  layer  {zo'na  denticula'ta),  and  which  carries  a  peculiar  elastic  pierced 
membrane,  Corti'B  membrane,  M.C.  fig.  49.  This  membrane  is  stretched  parallel 
to  the  membrana  basilaris  as  far  as  the  bony  wall  on  the  outer  side  of  the  duct, 
and  is  there  attached  a  little  above  the  other.  Between  these  two  membranes 
lie  the  parts  in  and  on  which  the  nerve  fibres  terminate. 

Among  these  Cortl's  arches  (over  g  in  fig.  49)  are  relatively  the  most  solid 
formations.      The  series  of  these  contiguous  arches  consists  of  two  series  of  rods 

*  [As  the  engraving  would  have  been  too  the  left  side  consequently  corresponds  to  the 
wide  for  the  page  if  placed  in  its  proper  hori-  upper,  and  its  right  to  the  wndcrsidc.— jTm/w- 
zontal  position^  it  has  been  printed  vertically;      lator.]  .     ,,    i     OOOIC 


14© 


COCHLEA  OF  THE  EAR. 


PABT  I. 


W' 


OT  fibres,  an  external  and  an  internal.  A  single  pair  of  these  is  shewn  in  fig.  50, 
A,  below,  and  a  short  series  of  them  in  fig.  50,  B,  attached  to  the  membrana 
basilaris,  and  at  t  also  connected  with  the  pierced  ^,0.  ^ 

tissue,  into  which  fit  the  terminal  cells  of  the  nerves 
(fig.  49,  c),  which  will  be  more  folly  described  pre- 
sently. These  formations  are  shewn  in  fig.  51, 
(p.  1416,  c),  as  seen  from  the  vestibule  gallery  ;  a  is 
the  denticulated  layer,  c  the  openings  for  the 
nerves  on  the  internal  margin  of  the  membrana 
basilaris,  its  external  margin  being  visible  at  u  u  ; 
d  is  the  inner  series  of  Corti's  rods,  e  the  outor ; 
over  these,  between  e  and  x  is  seen  the  pierced 
membrane,  against  which  lie  the  terminal  cells  of 

%  the  nerves. 

The  fibres  of  the  first,  or  outer  series,  are  flat, 
somewhat  S-shaped  formations,  having  a  swelling 
at  the  spot  where  they  rise  from  the  membrane  to 
which  they  are  attached,  and  ending  in  a  kind  of 
articulation  which  serves  to  connect  them  with  the 
second  or  inner  seiies.  In  fig.  51,  p.  141,  at  d 
will  be  seen  a  great  number  of  these  ascending 
fibres,  lying  beside  each  other  in  regular  succession. 
In  the  same  way  they  may  be  seen  all  along  the 
membrane  of  the  cochlea,  close  together,  so  that 
there  must  be  many  thousands  of  them.  Their 
sides  lie  close  together,  and  even  seem  to  be  con- 
nected, leaving  however  occasional  gaps  in  the  line 

%  of  connection,  and  these  gaps  are  probably  tra- 
versed by  nerve  fibres.  Hence  the  fibres  of  the 
first  series  as  a  whole  form  a  stiff  layer,  which 
endeavours  to  erect  itself  when  the  natural  fasten- 
ings no  longer  resist,  but  allows  the  membrane  on 
which  they  stand  to  crumple  up  between  the  at- 
tachments d  and  e  of  Corti's  arches. 

The  fibres  of  the  second,  or  inner  series,  which 
form  the  descending  part  of  the  arch  e,  fig.  50, 
below,  are  smooth,  flexible,  cylindrical  threads 
with  thickened  ends.  The  upper  extremity  forms 
a  kind  of  joint  to  connect  them  with  the  fibres 
of  the  first  series,  the  lower  extremity  is  enlarged  in  a  bell  shape  and  is  attached 
closely  to  the  membrane  at  the  base.     In  the  microscopic  preparations  they  gene- 

H  Fro.  50. 

A  B 


A,  external  and  internal  rod  in  connection  seen  In  profile.  B,  membrana  basilaris  (b)  with  the 
terminal  fiiscTcQll  of  nerres  (n),  and  the  internal  and  external  rodft  (i  and  e).  i  internal, 
3  external  cells  of  the  floor,  4'  attachments  of  the  cells  of  the  corer.    *  *    cpith^lum. 

rally  appear  bent  in  various  ways ;  but  there  can  be  no  doubt  tliat  in  their  natural 
condition  they  are  stretched  with  some  degree  of  tension,  so  that  they  pull  down 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


CHAP.  VI. 


COCHLEA  OF  THE   EAR. 


141 


the  upper  jointed  ends  of  the  fibres  of  the  first  series.  The  fibres  of  the  first 
series  arise  from  the  inner  margin  of  the  membrane,  which  can  be  relatively  little 
agitated,  but  the  fibres  of  the  second  series  are  attached  nearly  in  the  middle  of  the 
membrane,  and  this  is  precisely  the  place  where  its  vibrations  will  have  the  greatest 
excursions.  When  the  pressure  of  the  fluid  in  the  drum  gallery  of  the  labyrinth 
is  increased  by  driving  the  foot  of  the  stirrup  against  the  oval  window,  the  mem- 
brane at  the  base  of  the  arches  will  sink  downwards,  the  fibres  of  the  second  series 
be  more  tightly  stretched,  and  perhaps  the  corresponding  places  of  the  fibres  of  the 
first  series  be  bent  a  little  downwards.  It  does  not,  however,  seem  probable  that 
the  fibres  of  the  first  series  themselves  move  to  any  great  extent,  for  their  lateral 
connections  are  strong  enough  to  make  them  hang  together  in  masses  like  a 
membrane,  when  they  have  been  released  from  their  attachment  in  anatomical 
preparations.  On  reviewing  the  whole  arrangement,  there  can  be  no  doubt  that 
Corti*s  organ  is  an  apparatus  adapted  for  receiving  the  vibrations  of  the  membrana  f 


rrTrr 


basilaris,  and  for  vibrating  of  itself,  but  our  present  knowledge  is  not  sufficient  to 
determine  with  accuracy  the  manner  in  which  these  vibrations  take  place.  For 
this  purpose  we  require  to  estimate  the  stability  of  the  several  parts  and  the  degree 
of  tension  and  flexibility,  with  more  precision  than  can  be  deduced  from  such 
observations  as  have  hitherto  been  made  on  isolated  parts,  as  they  casually  group 
themselves  under  the  microscope. 

Now  Corti's  fibres  are  wound  round  and  covered  over  with  a  multitude  of  very 
dehcate,  frail  formations,  fibres  and  cells  of  various  kinds,  partly  the  finest  ter- 
minational  runners  of  nerve  fibres  with  appended  nerve  cells,  partly  fibres  of  liga- 
mentous tissue,  which  appear  to  serve  as  a  support  for  fixing  and  suspending  the 
nerve  formations. 

The  connection  of  these  parts  is  best  shewn  in  fig.  49  opposite.  They  are 
grouped  like  a  pad  of  soft  c^lls  on  each  side  of  and  within  Corti's  arches.  The 
most  important  of  them  appear  to  be  the  cells  c  and  d,  which  are  furnished  with 

Digitized  by  V^ O OQ IC 


H 


142  DAMPING  OF  THE   VIBRATIONS  IN   THE   EAR.         parti. 

hairs,  precisely  resembling  the  ciliated  cells  in  the  ampullae  and  utriculus.    They 
appear  to  be  directly  connected  by  fine  varicose  nerve  fibres,  and  constitute  the 
most  constant  part  of  the  cochlean  formations ;  for  with  birds  and  reptiles,  where 
the  structure  of  the  cochlea  is  much  simpler,  and  even  Corti's  arches  are  absent, 
these  little  ciliated  cells  are  always  to  be  found,  and  their  hairs  are  so  placed  as  to 
strike  against  Corti*s  membrane  during  the  vibration  of  the  membrana  basilaris. 
The  cells  at  a  and  a',  fig.  49  (p.  140),  which  appear  in  an  enlarged  condition  at  b 
and  n  in  fig.  51  (p.  141),  seem  to  have  the  character  of  an  epithelium.     In  fig.  51 
there  will  also  be  observed  bundles  and  nets  of  fibres,  which  may  be  partly  merely 
supporting  fibres  of  a  ligamentous  nature,  and  may  partly,  to  judge  by  their  appear- 
ance as  strings  of  beads,  possess  the  character  of  bundles  of  the  finest  fibriles  of 
nerves.    But  these  parts  are  all  so  frail  and  delicate  that  there  is  still  much 
doubt  as  to  their  connection  and  office, 
f       The  essential  result  of  our  description  of  the  ear  may  consequently  be  said  to 
consist  in  having  found  the  terminations  of  the  auditory  nerves  everywhere  con- 
nected with  a  pecuhar  auxiliary  apparatus,  partly  elastic,  partly  firm,  which  may  be 
put  in  sympathetic  vibration  under  the  influence  of  external  vibration,  and  will  then 
probably  agitate  and  excite  the  mass  of  nerves.    Now  it  was  shewn  in  Chap.  HI., 
that  the  process  of  sympathetic  vibration  was  observed  to  differ  according  as  the 
bodies  put  into  sympathetic  vibration  were  such  as  when  once  put  in  motion  con- 
tinued to  sound  for  a  long  time,  or  soon  lost  their  motion,  p.  39c.     Bodies  which, 
like  timing-forks  when  once  struck,  go  on  sounding  for  a  long  time,  are  susceptible 
of  sympathetic  vibration  in  a  high  degree  notwithstanding  the  difficulty  of  putting 
their  mass  in  motion,  because  they  admit  of  a  long  accumulation  of  impulses  in 
themselves  minute,  produced  in  them  by  each  separate  vibration  of  the  exciting 
tone.    But  precisely  for  this  reason  there  must  be  the  exactest  agreement  between 
the  pitches  of  the  proper  tone  of  the  fork  and  of  the  exciting  tone,  because  other- 
%  wise  subsequent  impulses  given  by  the  motion  of  the  air  could  not  constantly  recur 
in  the  same  phase  of  vibration,  and  thus  be  suitable  for  increasing  the  subsequent 
effect  of  the  preceding  impulses.    On  the  other  hand  if  we  take  bodies  for  which 
the  tone  rapidly  dies  away,  such  as  stretched  membranes  or  thin  light  strings,  we 
find  that  they  are  not  only  susceptible  of  S3rmpathetic  vibration,  when  vibrating 
air  is  allowed  to  act  on  them,  but  that  this  sympathetic  vibration  is  not  so  limited 
to  a  particular  pitch,  as  in  the  other  case,  and  they  can  therefore  be  easily  set  in 
motion  by  tones  of  different  kinds.    For  if  an  elastic  body  on  being  once  struck 
and  allowed  to  sound  freely,  loses  nearly  the  whole  of  its  motion  after  ten  vibra- 
tions, it  will  not  be  of  much  importance  that  any  fresh  impulses  received  after  the 
expiration  of  this  time,  should  agree  exactly  with  the  former,  although  it  would  be 
of  great  importance  in  the  case  of  a  sonorous  body  for  which  the  motion  generated 
by  the  first  impulse  would  remain  nearly  unchanged  up  to  the  time  that  the  second 
impulse  was  apphed.    In  the  latter  case  the  second  impulse  could  not  increase  the 
^  amount  of  motion,  unless  it  came  upon  a  phase  of  the  vibration  which  had 
pi'eeisely  the  same  direction  of  motion  as  itself. 

The  coimection  between  these  two  relations  can  be  calculated  independently  of 
the  nature  of  the  body  put  into  sympathetic  vibration,*  and  as  the  results  are  im- 
portant to  enable  us  to  form  a  judgment  on  the  state  of  things  going  on  in  the  ear, 
a  short  table  is  annexed.  Suppose  that  a  body  which  vibrates  sympathetically  has 
been  set  into  its  state  of  maximum  vibration  by  means  of  an  exact  unison,  and 
that  the  exciting  tone  is  then  altered  till  the  sympathetic  vibration  is  reduced  to 
^  of  its  former  amount.  The  amount  of  the  required  difference  of  pitch  is  given  in 
the  first  column  in  terms  of  an  equally  tempered  Tone,  [which  is  I  of  an  Octave]. 
Now  let  the  same  sonorous  body  be  struck,  and  let  its  sound  be  allowed  to  die 
away  gradually.  The  number  of  vibrations  which  it  has  made  by  the  time  that  its 
intensity  is  reduced  to  ^  of  its  original  amount  is  noted,  and  given  in  the  second 
column. 

*  The  mode  of  calculation  in  explained  in  Appendix  X. 


Digitized  by  VjOOQIC 


CHAP.  VI.      DAMPING  OF  THE  VIBRATIONS  IN  THE  EAR. 


143 


IMfference  of  Pitch,  in  terms  of  an  eqnally  tempered  Tone,  neces- 

maj  to  rednoe  the  intensity  of  sympathetic  Tlbnition  to  ^  of  that 

luroduoed  by  perfect  uuisonance 

Number  of  vibrations  after  which 
the  intensity  of  tone  in  a  sonorous 
body  whooe  sound  is  allowed  to  die 
out,  reduces  to  t^  of  its  original 
amount 

1.  One  eighth  of  a  Tone 

2.  One  quarter  of  a  Tone 

3.  One  Semitone 

4.  Three  quarters  of  a  Tone 

5.  A  whole  Tone 

6.  A  Tone  and  a  quarter 

7.  A  tempered  minor  Third  or  a  Tone  and  a  half. . 

8.  A  Tone  and  three  quarters 

9.  A  tempered  major  Third  or  two  whole  Tones     . 

3800 

19*00 

950 

6-33 

3-8o 

3*17 
271 

2-37 

Now,  although  we  are  not  able  exactly  to  discover  how  long  the  ear  and  its 
individual  parts,  when  set  in  motion,  will  continue  to  sound,  yet  well-known  «| 
experiments  allow  us  to  form  some  sort  of  judgment  as  to  the  position  which 
the  parts  of  the  ear  must  occupy  in  the  scale  exhibited  in  this  table.  Thus,  there 
cannot  possibly  be  any  parts  of  the  ear  which  continue  to  sound  so  long  as  a 
tuning-fork,  for  that  would  be  patent  to  the  commonest  observation.  But  even  if 
there  were  any  parts  in  the  ear  answering  to  the  first  degree  of  our  table,  that  is 
requiring  38  vibrations  to  be  reduced  to  -jJ^  of  their  force, — we  should  recognise 
this  in  the  deeper  tones,  because  38  vibrations  last  ^  of  a  second  tor  A,  ^  for  a, 
^  for  a\  &c.,  and  such  a  long  endurance  of  sensible  sound  would  render  rapid 
musical  passages  impossible  in  the  unaccented  and  once-accented  Octaves.  Such 
a  state  of  things  would  disturb  musical  effect  as  much  as  the  strong  resonance  of 
a  vaulted  room,  or  as  raising  the  dampers  on  a  piano.  When  making  a  shake,  we 
can  readily  strike  8  or  10  notes  in  a  second,  so  that  each  tone  separately  is  struck 
from  4  to  5  times.  If,  then,  the  sound  of  the  first  tone  had  not  died  off  in  our  ear 
before  the  end  of  the  second  sound,  at  least  to  such  an  extent  as  not  to  be  sensible  ^r 
when  the  latter  was  sounding,  the  tones  of  the  shake,  instead  of  being  individually 
distinct,  would  merge  into  a  continuous  mixture  of  both.  Now  shakes  of  this  kind, 
with  10  tones  to  a  second,  can  be  clearly  and  sharply  executed  throughout  almost 
the  whole  scale,  although  it  must  be  owned  that  from  A  downwards,  in  the  great 
and  contra  Octaves  they  sound  bad  and  rough,  and  their  tones  begin  to  mix.  Yet 
it  can  be  easily  shewn  that  this  is  not  due  to  the  mechanism  of  the  instrument. 
Thus  if  we  execute  a  shake  on  the  harmonium,  the  keys  of  tlie  lower  notes  are 
just  as  accurately  constructed  and  just  as  easy  to  move  as  those  of  the  higher 
ones.  Each  separate  tone  is  completely  cut  off  with  perfect  certainty  at  the 
moment  the  valve  &lls  on  the  air  passage,  and  each  speaks  at  the  moment  the  valve 
is  rfdsed,  because  during  so  brief  an  interruption  the  tongues  remain  in  a  state  of 
vibration.  Similarly  for  the  violoncello.  At  the  instant  when  the  finger  which 
makes  the  shake  &lls  on  the  string,  the  latter  must  commence  a  vibration  of  a 
different  periodic  time,  due  to  its  length ;  and  the  instant  that  the  finger  is  m 
removed,  the  vibration  belonging  to  the  deeper  tone  must  return.  And  yet  the 
shake  in  the  bass  is  as  imperfect  on  the  violoncello  as  on  any  other  instrument. 
Bans  and  shakes  can  be  relatively  best  executed  on  a  pianoforte  because,  at  the 
moment  of  striking,  the  new  tone  soimds  with  great  but  rapidly  decreasing  inten- 
sity. Hence,  in  addition  to  the  inharmonic  noise  produced  by  the  simultaneous 
continuance  of  the  two  tones,  we  also  hear  a  distinct  prominence  given  to  each 
separate  tone.  Now,  since  the  difficulty  of  shaking  in  the  bass  is  the  same  for  all 
instruments,  and  for  individual  instruments  is  demonstrably  independent  of  the 
manner  in  which  the  tones  are  produced,  we  are  forced  to  conclude  that  the 
difficulty  lies  in  the  ear  itself.  We  have,  then,  a  plain  indication  that  the  vibrating 
parts  of  the  ear  are  not  damped  with  sufficient  force  and  rapidity  to  allow  of 
successfully  effecting  such  a  rapid  alternation  of  tones. 

Nay  more,  this  fact  further  proves  that  there  must  be  different  parts  of  the  ear 
vrhich  are  set  in  vihration  by  tones  of  different  2)itch  and  which  receive  the  sejisntion 

Digitized  by  V^OOQIC 


144  DAMPING   OF  THE   VIBRATIONS  IN  THE   EAE.         parti. 

of  these  tones.  Thus,  it  might  be  supposed  that  as  the  vibratory  mass  of  the  -whole 
ear,  the  drumskin,  auditory  ossicles,  and  fluid  in  the  labyrinth,  were  vibratinf?  at 
tlie  same  time,  the  inertia  of  this  mass  was  the  cause  why  the  sonorous  vibrations 
in  the  ear  were  not  immediately  extinguished.  But  this  hypothesis  would  not 
suffice  to  explain  the  &ct  observed.  For  an  elastic  body  set  into  sympathetic 
vibration  by  any  tone,  vibrates  sympathetically  in  the  pitch  number  of  the  exciting 
tone ;  but  as  soon  as  the  exciting  tone  ceases,  it  goes  on  sounding  in  the  pitch 
number  of  its  own  proper  tone.  This  fact,  which  is  derived  from  theory,  may  be 
perfectly  verified  on  tuning-forks  by  means  of  the  vibration  microscope. 

If,  then,  the  ear  vibrated  as  a  single  system,  and  were  capable  of  continuing 
its  vibration  for  a  sensible  time,  it  would  have  to  do  so  with  its  own  pitch  number, 
which  is  totally  independent  of  the  pitch  number  of  the  former  exciting  tone. 
The  consequence  is  that  shakes  would  be  equally  difficult  upon  both  high  and 

^'  low  tones,  and  next  that  the  two  tones  of  the  shake  would  not  mix  with  each 
other,  but  that  each  would  mix  with  a  third  tone,  due  to  the  ear  itself.  We  became 
acquainted  with  such  a  tone  in  the  last  chapter,  the  high  /'",  p.  1 1 6a.  The  result, 
then,  under  these  circumstances  would  be  quite  different  from  what  is  observed. 

Now  if  a  shake  of  lo  notes  in  a  second,  be  made  on  ^,  of  which  the  vibra- 
tional number  is  no,  this  tone  would  be  struck  every  Ji  of  a  second.  We  may 
justly  assume  that  the  shake  would  not  be  clear,  if  the  intensity  of  the  expiring 
tone  were  not  reduced  to  ^^  of  its  original  amount  in  this  ^  of  a  second.  In  this 
case,  after  at  least  22  vibrations,  the  parts  of  the  ear  which  vibrate  sympathetically 
with  A  must  descend  to  at  least  ^V  ^^  their  intensity  of  vibration  as  their  tone 
expires,  so  that  their  power  of  sympathetic  vibration  cannot  be  of  the  first  degree 
in  the  table  on  p.  143a,  but  may  belong  to  the  second,  third,  or  some  other  higher 
degree.  That  the  degree  cannot  be  any  much  higher  one,  is  shewn  in  the  first 
place  by  the  fact  that  shakes  and  runs  begin  to  be  difficult  even  on  tones  which  do 

^  not  lie  much  lower.  This  we  shall  see  by  observations  on  beats  subsequently  de- 
tailed. We  may  on  the  whole  assume  that  the  parts  of  the  ear  which  vibrate 
sympathetically  have  an  amount  of  damping  power  corresponding  to  the  third 
degree  of  our  table,  where  the  intensity  of  sympathetic  vibration  with  a  Semitone 
difference  of  pitch  is  only  ^  of  what  it  is  for  a  complete  unison.  Of  course  there 
can  be  no  question  of  exact  determinations,  but  it  is  important  for  us  to  be  able 
to  form  at  least  an  approximate  conception  of  the  influence  of  damping  on  the 
sympathetic  vibration  of  the  ear,  as  it  has  great  significance  in  tlie  relations 
of  consonance.  Hence  when  we  hereafter  speak  of  individual  parts  of  the  ear 
vibrating  sympathetically  with  a  determinate  tone,  we  mean  that  they  are  set  into 
strongest  motion  by  that  tone,  but  are  also  set  into  vibration  less  strongly  by  tones 
of  nearly  the  same  pitch,  and  that  this  sympathetic  vibration  is  still  sensible  for 
the  interval  of  a  Semitone.  Fig.  52  may  serve 
to  give  a  general  conception  of  the  law  by  which 

If  the  intensity  of  the  sympathetic  vibration  de- 
creases, as  the  difference  of  pitch  increases.  The 
horizontal  line  a  b  c  represents  a  portion  of  the 
musical  scale,  each  of  the  lengths  a  b  and  b  c 
standing  for  a  whole  (equally  tempered)  Tone. 
Suppose  that  the  body  which  vibrates  sympa- 
thetically has  been  tuned  to  the  tone  b  and  that 
the  vertical  line  b  d  represents  the  maximum 
of  intensity  of  tone  which  it  can  attain  when  excited  by  a  tone  in  perfect  unison 
with  it.  On  the  base  line,  intervals  of  ^^^  of  a  whole  Tone  are  set  off,  and  the  ver- 
tical lines  drawn  through  them  shew  the  corresponding  intensity  of  the  tone  in  the 
body  which  vibrates  sympathetically,  when  the  exciting  tone  differs  from  a  unison 
by  the  corresponding  interval.  The  following  are  the  numbers  from  which  fig.  52 
was  constructed : — 


Digitized  by 


Google 


CHAP.  VI.      THEOBY  OP  THE  FUNCTION  OP  THE  COCHLEA. 


145 


Difleronce  of  Pitch 

Intensity 
of  Sympathetio  Vibration 

Difference  of  Pitch 

Intensity 
of  Sympathetic  VibraUon 

0*0 
O'l 
0-2 
0-3 

0-4 
Semitone 

lOO 

74 
41 
24 
15 

10 

0-6 
Whole  Tone 

7-2 

5-4 
4-2 

3*3 
2-7 

Now  we  cannot  precisely  ascertain  what  parts  of  the  ear  actually  vibrate  sym- 
pathetically with  individual  tones.*  We  can  only  conjecture  what  they  are  at 
present  in  the  case  of  human  beings  and  mammals.  The  whole  construction  of 
the  partition  of  the  cochlea,  and  of  Corti's  arches  which  rest  upon  it,  appears  most 
suited  for  executing  independent  vibrations.  We  do  not  need  to  require  of  them 
the  power  of  continuing  their  vibrations  for  a  long  time  without  assistance.  ^ 

But  if  these  formations  are  to  serve  for  distinguishing  tones  of  dififerent  pitch, 
and  if  tones  of  different  pitch  are  to  be  equally  well  perceived  in  all  parts  of  the 
scale,  the  elastic  formations  in  the  cochlea,  which  are  connected  with  different 
nerve  fibres,  must  be  differently  tuned,  and  their  proper  tones  must  form  a  regu- 
larly progressive  series  of  degrees  through  the  whole  extent  of  the  musical  scale. 

According  to  the  recent  anatomical  researches  of  V.  Hensen  and  G.  Hasse,  it 
is  probably  the  breadth  of  the  membrana  basilaris  in  the  cochlea,  which  deter- 
mines the  tuning.f  At  its  commencement  opposite  the  oval  window,  it  is 
comparatively  narrow,  and  it  continually  increases  in  width  as  it  approaches  the 
apex  of  the  cochlea.  The  following  measurements  of  the  membrane  in  a  newly 
bom  child,  from  the  line  where  the  nerves  pass  through  on  the  inner  edge,  to  the 
attachment  to  the  ligamentum  spirale  on  the  outer  edge,  are  given  by  V.  Hensen : 


Place  of  Section 

Breadth  of  Membnuie  or  Length  of  Trans- 
verse Fibres 

Millimetres 

Inches 

0-2625  mm.  [«  0-010335  i»-J  from  root    . 
0*8626  mm.  [=0*033961  in.J  from  root    . 

Middle  of  the  first  spire 

End  of  first  spire 

Middle  of  second  spire     ...... 

End  of  second  spire 

At  the  hamalos 

0-04125 

0-0825 

0-169 

0-3 

0-4125 

0-45 

0-495 

•00162 
•00325 
•00665 
•OII81 
•01624 
•01772 
•01949 

The  breadth  therefore  increases  more  than  twelvefold  from  the  beginning  to 
the  end. 

Corti's  rods  also  exhibit  an  increase  of  size  as  they  approach  the  vertex  of  the 
cochlea,  but  in  a  much  less  degree  than  the  membrana  basilaris.  The  following 
are  Hensen's  measurements  : — 


at  the  round  window 

at  the  hunolus 

nun. 

inch 

mm. 

inch 

Length  of  inner  rod  . 

Length  of  outer  rod  .... 

Span  of  the  arch       .... 

0-048 
0-048 
0*019 

000189 
000189 
0-00075 

fit 

0-C0337 
0-00386 
0-00335 

*  [Here  the  passage,  *The  particles  of 
anditory  sand,'  to  'used  for  musical  tones,* 
on  pp.  217-18  of  the  ist  English  edition  has 
been  cancelled,  and  the  passage  *W6  can  only 
oonjectnre,'  to  *  without  assistance,'  on  p.  145a 
added  in  its  place  from  the  4th  German  edition. 
— Translator.'] 

t  In  the  1st  [German]  edition  of  this  book 
(1863),  which  was  written  at  a  time  when  the 
more  delicate  anatomy  of  the  cochlea  was  just 


beginning  to  be  developed,  I  supposed  thai  the 
different  degrees  of  stiffness  and  tension  in 
Corti's  rods  themselves  might  furnish  the 
reason  of  their  different  tuning.  By  Hensen's 
measures  of  the  breadth  of  the  membrana 
basilaris  {Ztitschrift  filr  wissenseh.  Zoologie, 
voL  ziii.  p.  492)  and  Hasse's  proof  that  Corti's 
rods  are  absent  in  birds  and  amphibia,  far  more 
definite  foundations  for  forming  a  judgment 
have  been  furnished,  than  I  then  possessed. 


Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


146  THEORY  OP  THE   FUNCTION  OF  THE  COCHLEA.      part  u 

Hence  it  follows,  as  Henle  has  also  proved,  that  the  greatest  increase  of  breadth 
falls  on  the  outer  zone  of  the  basilar  membrane,  beyond  the  line  of  the  attach- 
ment of  the  outer  rods.  This  increases  from  0*023  mm.  [='000905  in.]  to  0*41 
nun.  [= -016 142  inch]  or  nearly  twentyfold. 

In  accordance  with  these  measures,  the  two  rows  of  Corti's  rods  are  almost 
parallel  and  upright  near  to  the  roimd  window,  but  they  are  bent  much  more 
strongly  towards  one  another  near  the  vertex  of  the  cochlea. 

It  has  been  already  mentioned  that  the  membrana  basilaris  of  the  cochlea 
breaks  easily  in  the  radial  direction,  but  that  its  radial  fibres  have  considerable 
tenacity.  This  seems  to  me  to  furnish  a  very  important  mechanical  relation, 
namely  that  this  membrane  in  its  natural  connection  admits  of  being  tightly 
stretched  in  the  transverse  direction  from  the  modiolus  to  the  outer  wall  of  the 
cochlea,  but  can  have  only  little  tension  in  the  direction  of  its  length,  because  it 
^  could  not  resist  a  strong  puU  in  this  direction. 

Now  the  mathematical  theory  of  the  vibration  of  a  membrane  with  different  ten- 
sions in  different  directions  shews  that  it  behaves  very  differently  from  a  membrane 
which  has  the  same  tension  in  all  directions.*  On  the  latter,  vibrations  produced 
in  one  part,  spread  uniformly  in  all  directions,  and  hence  if  the  tension  were  uniform 
it  would  be  impossible  to  set  one  part  of  the  basilar  membrane  in  vibration,  without 
producing  nearly  as  strong  vibrations  (disregarding  individual  nodal  lines)  in  all  other 
parts  of  the  membrane. 

But  if  the  tension  in  direction  of  its  length  is  infinitesimally  small  in  com- 
parison with  the  tension  in  direction  of  the  breadth,  then  the  radial  fibres  of 
the  basilar  membrane  may  be  approximatively  regarded  as  forming  a  system  of 
stretched  strings,  and  the  membranous  connection  as  only  serving  to  give  a  ful- 
crum to  the  pressure  of  the  fluid  against  these  strings.  In  that  case  the  laws  of 
their  motion  would  be  the  same  as  if  every  individual  string  moved  independently 
4|[  of  all  the  others,  and  obeyed,  by  itself,  the  influence  of  the  periodically  alternating 
pressure  of  the  fluid  of  the  labyrinth  contained  in  the  vestibule  gallery.  Conse- 
quently any  exciting  tone  would  set  that  part  of  the  membrane  into  sympathetic 
vibration,  for  which  the  proper  tone  of  one  of  its  radial  fibres  that  are  stretched 
and  loaded  with  the  various  appendages  already  described,  corresponds  most  nearly 
with  the  exciting  tone  ;  and  thence  the  vibrations  will  extend  with  rapidly  dimi- 
nishing strength  on  to  the  adjacent  parts  of  the  membrane.  Fig.  52,  on  p.  14402, 
might  be  taken  to  represent,  on  an  exaggerated  scale  of  height,  a  longitudinal  sec- 
tion of  that  part  of  the  basilar  membrane  in  which  the  proper  tone  of  the  radial 
fibres  of  the  membrane  are  nearest  to  the  exciting  tone. 

The  strongly  vibrating  parts  of  the  membrane  would,  as  has  been  explained  in 
respect  to  all  bodies  which  vibrate  sympathetically,  be  more  or  less  limited,  accord- 
ing to  the  degree  of  damping  power  in  the  adjacent  parts,  by  friction  against  the 
fluid  in  the  labyrinth  and  in  the  soft  gelatinous  parts  of  the  nerve  fillet. 
^  Under  these  circumstances  the  parts  of  the  membrane  in  unison  with  higher 
tones  must  be  looked  for  near  the  round  window,  and  those  with  the  deeper,  near 
the  vertex  of  the  cochlea,  as  Hensen  also  concluded  from  his  measurements.  That 
such  short  strings  should  be  capable  of  corresponding  with  such  deep  tones,  must 
be  explained  by  their  being  loaded  in  the  basilar  membrane  with  all  kinds  of  solid 
formations  ;  the  fluid  of  both  galleries  in  the  cochlea  must  also  be  considered  as 
weighting  the  membrane,  because  it  cannot  move  without  a  kind  of  wave  motion 
in  that  fluid. 

The  observations  of  Hasse  shew  that  Corti's  arches  do  not  exist  in  the  cochlea 
of  birds  and  amphibia,  although  the  other  essential  parts  of  the  cochlea,  as  the 
basilar  membrane,  the  ciliated  cells  in  coimection  with  the  terminations  of  the 
nerves,  and  Corti's  membrane,  which  stands  opposite  the  ends  of  these  ciliae,  are 
all  present.  Hence  it  becomes  very  probable  that  Corti's  arches  play  only  a 
secondary  part  in  the  function  of  the  cochlea.    Perhaps  we  might  look  for  the  effect 

*  See  Appendix  XI. 

Digitized  by  VjOOQIC 


CHAP.  VI.      THEORY  OF  THE   FUNCTION   OF  THE   COCHLEA.         147 

of  Corti's  arches  in  their  power,  as  relatively  firm  objects,  of  transmitting  the 
vibrations  of  the  basilar  membrane  to  small  limited  regions  of  the  upper  part  of 
the  relatively  thick  nervous  fillet,  better  than  it  could  be  done  by  the  immediate 
communication  of  the  vibrations  of  the  basilar  membrane  through  the  soft  mass 
of  this  fillet.  Close  to  the  outside  of  the  upper  end  of  the  arch,  connected  with 
it  by  the  stiffer  fibriles  of  the  membrana  reticularis,  are  the  ciliated  cells  of  the 
nervous  fillet  (see  c  in  fig.  49,  p.  140).  In  birds,  on  the  other  hand,  the  ciliated  cells 
form  a  thin  stratum  upon  the  basilar  membrane,  and  this  stratum  can  readily 
receive  limited  vibrations  from  the  membrane,  without  communicating  them  too 
&r  sideways. 

According  to  this  view  Corti's  arches,  id  the  last  resort,  will  be  the  means  of 
transmitting  the  vibrations  received  from  the  basilar  membrane  to  the  terminal 
appendages  of  the  conducting  nerve.  In  this  sense  the  reader  is  requested  here- 
after to  understand  references  to  the  vibrations,  proper  tone,  and  intonation  of  ^ 
Corti's  arches ;  the  intonation  meant  is  that  which  they  receive  through  their 
connection  with  the  corresponding  part  of  the  basilar  membrane. 

According  to  Waldeyer  there  are  about  4500  outer  arch  fibres  in  the  human 
cochlea.  If  we  deduct  300  for  the  simple  tones  which  lie  beyond  musical  limits, 
and  cannot  have  their  pitch  perfectly  apprehended,  there  remain  4200  for  the 
seven  octaves  of  musical  instruments,  that  is,  600  for  every  Octave,  50  for  every 
Semitone  [that  is,  i  for  every  2  cents] ;  certainly  quite  enough  to  explain  the 
power  of  distinguishing  small  parts  of  a  Semitone.*  According  to  Prof.  W. 
Preyer's  investigations,t  practised  musicians  can  distinguish  with  certainty  a 
difference  of  pitch  arising  from  half  a  vibration  in  a  second,  in  the  doubly 
accented  Octave.  This  would  give  1000  distinguishable  degrees  of  pitch  in  the 
Octave,  from  500  to  1000  vibrations  in  the  second.  Towards  the  Umits  of  the 
scale  the  power  to  distinguish  differences  diminishes.  The  4200  Corti'd  arches 
appear  then,  in  this  respect,  to  be  enough  to  apprehend  distinctions  of  thiscr 
amount  of  delicacy.  But  even  if  it  should  be  found  that  many  more  than 
4200  d^rees  of  pitch  could  be  distinguished  in  the  Octave,  it  would  not  prejudice 
our  assumption.  For  if  a  simple  tone  is  struck  having  a  pitch  between  those  of 
two  adjacent  Corti's  arches,  it  would  set  them  both  in  sympathetic  vibration,  and 
that  arch  would  vibrate  the  more  strongly  which  was  nearest  in  pitch  to  the 
proper  tone.  The  smallness  of  the  interval  between  the  pitches  of  two  fibres  still 
distinguishable,  will  therefore  finally  depend  upon  the  delicacy  with  which  the 
different  forces  of  the  vibrations  excited  can  be  compared.  And  we  have  thus 
also  an  explanation  of  the  fact  that  as  the  pitch  of  an  external  tone  rises  con- 
tinuously, our  sensations  also  alter  continuously  and  not  by  jumps,  as  must  be  the 
case  if  only  one  of  Corti's  arches  were  set  in  sympathetic  motion  at  once. 

To  draw  farther  conclusions  from  our  hypothesis,  when  a  simple  tone  is  pre- 
sented to  the  ear,  those  Corti's  arches  which  are  nearly  or  exactly  in  unison  with 
it  will  be  strongly  excited,  and  the  rest  only  slightly  or  not  at  aJl.    Hence  every  ir 
simple  tone  of  determinate  pitch  will  be  felt  only  by  certain  nerve  fibres,  and 

♦  [A  few  lines  of  the  ist  Engligh  edition  at  rib.          a  difference  of  orlitenralof 

have   here  been  cancelled,  and  replaced  by  S^           -300  vib.  ro  cents  1t/?a5  per- 

others  from  the  4th  German  edition.— Traw-  ^  '00°  ,         '5«>    ».  '9    ,.     jceived. 

l^ilfyr  1  but  on  the  other  hand 

« Jh7  (On  the  limits  of  the  perception    of  ,^  .^  ^'^-  6  cents    vaa 

tone),  June  1876.    Rearranged  in  English  by  "°  !^'    "  l^    "        ^^ 

the    Translator    in  the    Proceedings   of   the  r^  '^    "  '°    '»      P      ,, 

^T^u  iT^^!^  *!?'  H  ^^  v^*  PP-   V?^'  the  intervals  perceii^d.  or  not  ^r;;jiv^!  blSfg 

under  the  title  of  'On  the  Sensitiveness  of  the  ^^^  ^^^^y^^ ^^^^    diiferS^t     And  geL"^ 

EartoPitchandChangeofPitehm  Music/   On  ^^^j    throughout  the  scale  a  difference  of  fvib. 

p.  II  of  this  arrangement  It  is  stated  that.  .^  ^^  j^^^^l  ^^^  ^<^«      . 

includmg  Delezenne's  results.  ^^^^  q^  ^   .   a  ^y,  ■^ 

•t  Tib.  a  difference  of     or  Interral  of  __  j  *._„       '*   .    \^  J  I  .    ,^. 

^^  J  -r    t.  T    fi      ».  ~-Tra7islator.] 

Digitized  by  ^0©gie 


148 


THEORY  OP  THE  FUNCTION  OP  THE  COCHLEA.   pabt  i. 


1.T.O  \> 


7.1  h 


4 


simple  tones  of  different  pitch  will  excite  different  fibres.  When  a  compound 
musical  tone  or  chord  is  presented  to  the  ear,  all  those  elastic  bodies  will  be 
excited,  which  have  a  proper  pitch  corresponding  to  the  various  individual  simple 
tones  contained  in  the  whole  mass  of  tones,  and  hence  by  properly  directing 
attention,  all  the  individual  sensations  of  the  individual  simple  tones  can  be 
perceived.  The  chord  must  be  resolved  into  its  individual  compound  tones,  and 
the  compound  tone  into  its  individual  harmonic  partial  tones. 

This  also  explains  how  it  is  that  the  ear  resolves  a  motion  of  the  air  into 
pendular  vibrations  and  no  other.  Any  particle  of  air  can  of  course  execute  only 
one  motion  at  one  time.  That  we  considered  such  a  motion  mathematically  as  a 
sum  of  pendular  vibrations,  was  in  the  first  instance  merely  an  arbitrary  assump- 
tion to  facilitate  theory,  and  had  no  meaning  in  nature.  The  first  meaning  in 
nature  that  we  found  for  this  resolution  came  from  considering  sympathetic 

^vibration,  when  we  discovered  that  a  motion  which  was  not  pendular,  could 
produce  sympathetic  vibrations  in  bodies  of  those  different  pitches,  which  cor- 
responded to  the  harmonic  upper  partial  tones.  And  now  our  hypothesis  has  also 
reduced,  tjie  phenomenon  of  hearing  to  that  of  sympathetic  xJliration^and  thus 
furnished  a  reason  why  an  originally  simple  periodic  vibifttipn^  of  the  au:  pro- 
duces a  sum  of  different  sensations,  and  hence  also  a^tpeaxs  as  compound  to  our 
gerceptionp. 

The  sensation  of  different  pitch  would  consequently  be  a  sensation  in  different 
nerve  fibres.  The  sensation  of  a  quality  of  tone  would  depend  upon  the  power  of 
a  given  compound  tone  to  set  in  vibration  not  only  those  of  Corti*s  arches  which 
correspond  to  its  prime  tone,  but  also  a  series  of  other  arches,  and  hence  to  excite 
sensation  in  several  different  groups  of  nerve  fibres. 

Physiologically  it  should  be  observed  that  the  present  assumption  reduces 
sensations  which  differ  qualitatively  according  to  pitch  and  quality  of  tone,  to  a 

^  difference  in  the  nerve  fibres  which  are  excited.  This  is  a  step  similar  to  that 
taken  in  a  wider  field  by  Johannes  Miiller  in  his  theory  of  the  specific  energies  of 
sense.  He  has  shewn  that  the  difference  in  the  sensations  due  to  various  senses, 
does  not  depend  upon  the  actions  which  excite  them,  but  upon  the  various  nervous 
arrangements  which  receive  them.  We  can  convince  ourselves  experimentally 
that  in  whatever  manner  the  optic  nerve  and  its  expansion,  the  retina  of  the  eye, 
may  be  excited,  by  light,  by  twitching,  by  pressure,  or  by  electricity,  the  result  is 
never  anything  but  a  sensation  of  light,  and  that  the  tactual  nerves,  on  the  contrary, 
never  give  us  sensations  of  hght  or  of  hearing  or  of  taste.  The  same  solar  rays 
which  are  felt  as  light  by  the  eye,  are  felt  by  the  nerves  of  the  hand  as  heat ;  the 
same  agitations  which  are  felt  by  the  hand  as  twitterings,  are  tone  to  the  ear. 

Just  as  the  ear  apprehends  vibrations  of  different  periodic  time  as  tones  of 
different  pitch,  so  does  the  eye  perceive  luminiferous  vibrations  of  different  periodic 
time  as  different  colours,  the  quickest  giving  violet  and  blue,  the  mean  green  and 

^  yellow,  the  slowest  red.  The  laws  of  the  mixture  of  colours  led  Thomas  Young 
to  the  hypothesis  that  there  were  three  kinds  of  nerve  fibres  in  the  eye,  with 
different  powers  of  sensation,  for  feeling  red,  for  feeling  green,  and  for  feeling 
violet.  In  reality  this  assumption  gives  a  very  simple  and  perfectly  consistent 
explanation  of  all  the  optical  phenomena  depending  on  colour.  And  by  this  means 
the  qualitative  differences  of  the  sensations  of  sight  are  reduced  to  differences  in 

/the  nerves  which  receive  the  sensations.  For  the  sensations  of  each  individual 
fibre  of  the  optic  nerve  there  remains  only  the  quantitative  differences  of  greater  or 
less  irritation. 

The  same  result  is  obtained  for  hearing  by  the  hypothesis  to  which  our 
investigation  of  quality  of  tone  has  led  us.  The  qualitative  difference  of  pitch 
and  quality  of  tone  is  reduced  to  a  difference  in  the  fibres  of  the  nerve  receiving 
the  sensation,  and  for  each  individual  fibre  of  the  nerve  there  remains  only  the 
quantitative  difierences  in  the  amount  of  excitement. 

The  processes  of  irritation  within  the  nerves  of  the  muscles,  by  which  their 
contraction  is  determined,  have  hitherto  been 


more  accessi 

Digitized  ' 


?,^^t3d?gl^^°8^*^ 


CHAP.  VI.      THEORY  OF  THE  FUNCTION  OF  THE  COCHLEA.         149 

investigation  than  those  which  take  place  in  the  nerves  of  sense.    In  those  of  the 
muscle,  indeed,  we  find  only  quantitative  differencea  of- more  or  less  excitement, 
and  TU)  ^nftlifrfitivf^  jifTftrflt^(*<i>g  at  all.    In  them  we  are  able  to  establish,  that  during 
excitement  the  electrically  active  particles  of  the  nerves  undergo  determinate  (i 
changes,  and  that  these  changes  ensue  in  exactly  the  same  way  whatever  be  the  \\ 
excitement  which  causes  them.    But  precisely  the  same  changes  also  take  place  in  1 1 
an  excited  nerve  of  sense,  although  their  consequence  in  this  case  is  a  sensation, 
while  in  the  other  it  was  a  motion ;  and  hence  we  see  that  the  mechanism  of  the 
process  of  irritation  in  the  nerves  of  sense  must  be  m  every  respect_similar  to  that  j 
in  the  nerves  of  motion.     The  two*  hypotheses  just  explained  really  reduce  the     -2.Z2- 
processes  in  the  nerves  of  man's  two  principal  senses,  notwithstanding  their 
apparently  involved  qualitative  ^dilbiences  of  sensations,  to  the  same  simple 
gfiliprnft  ^K^^}\  Msrh\oh  wa  Ara  fftyniliflj  in  the  nerves  of  motioiu,   Nerves  have  been    r^^^J*^ 
often  and  not  unsuitably  compared  to  telegraph  wires.     Such  a  wire  conducts  one;1f 
kind  of  electric  current  and  no  other ;  it  may  be  stronger,  it  may  be  weaker,  it  may 
move  in  either  direction ;  it  has  no  other  quahtative  differences.    Nevertheless, 
according  to  the  different  kinds  of  apparatus  with  which  we  provide  its  termina- 
tions, we  can  send  telegraphic  despatches,  ring  bells,  explode  mines,  decompose 
water,  move  magnets,  magnetise  iron,  develop  hght,  and  so  on.     So  with  the 
nerves.     The  condition  of  excitement  which  can  be  produced  in  them,  and  is  con- 
ducted by  them,  is,  so  far  as  it  can  be  recognised  in  isolated  fibres  of  a  nerve, 
everywhere  the  same,  but  when  it  is  brought  to  various  parts  of  the  brain,  or 
the  body,  it  produces  motion,  secretions  of  glands,  increase  and  decrease  of  the 
quantity  of  blood,  of  redness  and  of  warmth  of  individual  organs,  and  also  sensa- 
tions of  light,  of  hearing,  and  so  forth.     Supposing  that  every  qualitatively 
different  action  is  produced  in  an  organ  of  a  different  kind,  to  which  also  separate 
fibres  of  nerve  must  proceed,  then  the  actual  process  of  irritation  in  individual 
nerves  may  always  be  precisely  the  same,  just  as  the  electrical  current  in  the  tele-  ^ 
graph  wires  remains  one  and  the  same  notwithstanding  the  various  kinds  of 
effects  which  it  produces  at  its  extremities.    On  the  other  hand,  if  we  assume  that 
the  same  fibre  of  a  nerve  is  capable  of  conducting  different  kinds  of  sensation,  we 
should  have  to  assume  that  it  admits  of  various  kinds  of  processes  of  irritation, 
and  this  we  have  been  hitherto  unable  to  establish. 

In  this  respect  then  the  view  here  proposed,  like  Young's  hypothesis  for  the 
difference  of  colours,  has  a  still  wider  signification  for  the  physiology  of  the 
nerves  in  general. 

Since  the  first  publication  of  this  book,  the  theory  of  auditory  sensation  here 
explained,  has  received  an  interesting  confirmation  from  the  observations  and 
experiments  made  by  V •  Hensen  *  on  the  auditory  apparatus  of  the  Crustaceae. 
These  animals  have  bags  of  auditory  stones  (otoUths),  partly  closed,  partly 
opening  outwards,  in  which  these  stones  float  freely  in  a  watery  fluid  and  are 
supported  by  hairs  of  a  peculiar  formation,  attached  to  the  stones  at  one  end,  and,  f 
partly,  arranged  in  a  series  proceeding  in  order  of  magnitude,  from  larger  and 
thicker  to  shorter  and  thinner.  In  many  crustaceans  also  we  find  precisely 
similar  hairs  on  the  open  surface  of  the  body,  and  these  must  be  considered  as 
auditory  hairs.  The  proof  that  these  external  hairs  are  also  intended  for  hearing, 
depends  first  on  the  similarity  of  their  construction  with  that  of  the  hairs  in  the 
bags  of  otoliths ;  and  secondly  on  Hensen's  discovery  that  the  sensation  of 
hearing  remained  in  the  Mysis  (opossum  shrimp)  when  the  bags  of  otoUths  had 
been  extirpated,  and  the  external  auditory  hairs  of  the  antennae  were  left. 

Hensen  conducted  the  sound  of  a  keyed  bugle  through  an  apparatus  formed  on 
the  model  of  the  drumskin  and  auditory  ossicles  of  the  ear  into  the  water  of  a 
little  box  in  which  a  specimen  of  Mysis  was  fastened  in  such  a  way  as  to  allow 
the  external  auditory  hairs  of  the  tail  to  be  observed.  It  was  then  seen  that 
certain  tones  of  the  horn  set  certain  hairs  into  strong  vibration,  and  other  tones 

*  Studien  ilber  das  Gehdrorgan  der  Deca-      and  lLb\\\\i%i*^  ZeUschrift  fUt  loissenselmftliche 

podcn,  Leipzig,  1863.    Benrinted  from  Siebold      Zooiooi^,  vol.  xiii.  •     r\r^rsi/> 

'  Digitized  by  VJwwVlC 


ISO  THEORY  OF  THE  FUNCTION  OF  THE  COCHLEA.      pabt  i; 

other  hairs.  Each  hair  answered  to  several  notes  of  the  horn,  and  from  the 
notes  mentioned  we  can  approximatively  recognise  the  series  of  under  tones  of  one 
and  the  same  simple  tone.  The  results  could  not  be  quite  exact,  because  the 
resonance  of  the  conducting  apparatus  must  have  had  some  influence. 

Thus  one  of  these  hairs  answered  strongly  to  djj^  and  d%  more  weakly  to  g, 
and  very  weakly  to  O.  This  leads  us  to  suppose  that  it  was  tuned  to  some  pitch 
between  d'^  and  d'%  In  that  case  it  answered  to  the  second  partial  of  d'  to  d'jj^ 
the  third  of  g  to  g$,  the  fourth  of  d  to  djj^,  and  the  sixth  of  G  to  (r$.  A  second 
hair  answered  strongly  to  aj{l  and  the  adjacent  tones,  more  weakly  to  djj^  and  Ajj^ 
Its  proper  tone  therefore  seems  to  have  been  a$. 

By  these  observations  (which  through  the  kindness  of  Herr  Hensen  I  have 
myself  had  the  opportunity  of  verifying)  the  existence  of  such  relations  as  we  have 
supposed  in  the  case  of  the  human  cochlea,  have  been  directly  proved  for  these 
f  Crustaceans,  and  this  is  the  more  valuable,  because  the  concealed  position  and 
ready  destructibility  of  the  corresponding  organs  of  the  human  ear  give  little  hope 
of  our  ever  being  able  to  make  such  a  direct  experiment  on  the  intonation  of  its 
individual  parts.* 

So  far  the  theory  which  has  been  advanced  refers  in  the  first  place  only  to 
the  lasting  sensation  produced  by  regular  and  continued  periodical  oscillations. 
But  as  regards  the  perception  of  irregular  motions  of  the  mr,  that  is,  of  noises,  it 
is  clear  that  an  elastic  apparatus  for  executing  vibrations  could  not  remain  at 
absolute  rest  in  the  presence  of  any  force  acting  upon  it  for  a  time,  and  even  a 
momentary  motion  or  one  recurring  at  irregular  intervals  would  suffice,  if  only 
powerful  enough,  to  set  it  in  motion.  The  peculiar  advantage  of  resonance  over 
proper  tone  depends- precisely  on  the  fact  that  disproportionately  weak  individual 
impulses,  provided  that  they  succeed  each  other  in  correct  rhythm,  are  capable  of 
producing  comparatively  considerable  motions.  On  the  other  hand,  momentary 
^  but  strong  impulses,  as  for  example  those  which  result  from  an  electric  spark,  will 
set  every  part  of  the  basilar  membrane  into  an  almost  equally  powerful  initial 
motion,  after  which  each  part  would  die  off  in  its  own  proper  vibrational  period. 
By  that  means  there  might  arise  a  simultaneous  excitement  of  the  whole  of  the 
nerves  in  the  cochlea,  which  although  not  equally  powerful  would  yet  be  propor- 
tionately gradated,  and  hence  could  not  have  the  character  of  a  determinate  pitch. 
Even  a  weak  impression  on  so  many  nerve  fibres  wiU  produce  a  clearer  impression 
than  any  single  impression  in  itself.  We  know  at  least  that  small  differences  of 
brightness  are  more  readily  perceived  on  large  than  on  small  parts  of  the  circle  of 
vision,  and  little  differences  of  temperature  can  be  better  perceived  by  plunging 
the  whole  arm,  than  by  merely  dipping  a  finger,  into  the  warm  water. 

Hence  a  perception  of  momentary  impulses  by  the  cochlear  nerves  is  quite 
possible,  just  as  noises  are  perceived,  without  giving  an  especially  sensible  pro- 
minence to  any  determinate  pitch. 
If  If  the  pressure  of  the  air  which  bears  on  the  drumskin  lasts  a  little  longer,  it 
will  favour  the  motion  in  some  regions  of  the  basilar  membrane  in  preference  to 
other  parts  of  the  scale.  Certain  pitches  will  therefore  be  especially  prominent. 
This  we  may  conceive  thus  :  every  instant  of  pressure  is  considered  as  a  pressure 
that  will  excite  in  every  fibre  of  the  basilar  membrane  a  motion  corresponding 
to  itself  in  direction  and  strength  and  then  die  off;  and  all  motions  in  each 
fibre  which  are  thus  excited  are  added  algebraically,  whence,  according  to  cir- 
cumstances, they  reinforce  or  enfeeble  each  other.t  Thus  a  uniform  pressure 
which  lasts  during  the  first  half  vibration,  that  is,  as  long  as  the  first  positive 
excursion,  increases  the  excursion  of  the  vibrating  body.  But  if  it  lasts  longer 
it  weakens  the  effect  first  produced.  Hence  rapidly  vibrating  bodies  would  be 
proportionably  less  excited  by  such  a  pressure,  than  those  for  which  half  a  vibra- 
tion lasts  as  long  as,  or  longer  than,  the  pressure  itself.     By  this  means  such  an 

*  [From  here  to  the  end  of  this  chapter  is  f  See  the  mathematical  expression  for  this 

an  addition  from  the  4th  German  edition,—      conception  at  the  end  of  Appendix  XI. 

Trajislaior.' 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQiC 


CHAP.  VI.      THEORY  OF  THE  FUNCTION  OF  THE  COCHLEA.         151 

impression  would  acquire  a  certain,  though  an  ill-defined,  pitch.  In  general  the 
intensity  of  the  sensation  seems,  for  an  equal  amount  of  vis  viva  in  the  motion,  to 
increase  as  the  pitch  ascends.  So  that  the  impression  of  the  highest  strongly 
excited  fibre  preponderates. 

A  determinate  pitch,  to  a  more  remarkable  extent,  may  also  naturally  result,  if 
the  pressure  itself  which  acts  on  the  stirrup  of  the  drum  alternates  several  times 
between  positive  and  negative.  And  thus  all  transitional  degrees  between  noises 
without  any  determinate  pitch,  and  compound  tones  with  a  determinate  pitch  may 
be  produced.  This  actually  takes  place,  and  herein  lies  the  proof,  on  which  Herr 
8.  Exner*  has  properly  laid  weight,  that  such  noises  must  be  perceived  by  those 
parts  of  the  ear  which  act  in  distinguishing  pitch. 

In  former  editions  of  this  work  I  had  expressed  a  conjecture  that  the  auditory 
dliae  of  the  ampullae,  which  seemed  to  be  but  Uttle  adapted  for  resonance,  and 
those  of  the  Httle  bags  opposite  the  otoliths,  might  be  especially  active  in  the  ^ 
perception  of  noises. 

As  regards  the  ciliae  in  the  ampullae,  the  investigations  of  Goltz  have  made  it 
extremely  probable  that  they,  as  well  as  the  semicircular  canals,  serve  for  a  totally 
different  kind  of  sensation,  namely  for  the  perception  of  the  turning  of  the  head. 
Revolution  about  an  axis  perpendicular  to  the  plane  of  one  of  the  semicircular 
canals  cannot  be  immediately  transferred  to  the  ring  of  water  which  lies  in  the 
canal,  and  on  account  of  its  inertia  lags  behind,  while  the  relative  shifting  of  the 
water  along  the  wall  of  the  canal  might  be  felt  by  the  ciliae  of  the  nerves  of  the 
ampullae.  On  the  other  hand,  if  the  turning  continues,  the  ring  of  water  itself 
will  be  gradually  set  in  revolution  by  its  friction  against  the  wall  of  the  canal, 
and  wiU  continue  to  move,  even  when  the  turning  of  the  head  suddenly  ceases. 
This  causes  the  illusive  sensation  of  a  revolution  in  the  contrary  direction,  in  the 
well-known  form  of  giddiness.  Injuries  to  the  semicircular  canals  without  injuries 
to  the  brain  produce  the  most  remarkable  disturbances  of  equihbrium  in  the  lower  ^ 
animals.  Electrical  discharges  through  the  ear  and  cold  water  squirted  into  the 
ear  of  a  person  vdth  a  perforated  drumskin,  produce  the  most  violent  giddiness. 
Under  these  circumstances  these  parts  of  the  ear  can  no  longer  with  any  probability 
be  considered  as  belonging  to  the  sense  of  hearing.  Moreover  impulses  of  the 
stirrup  against  the  water  of  the  labyrinth  adjoining  the  oval  window  are  in  reahty 
ill  adapted  for  producing  streams  through  the  semicircular  canals. 

On  the  other  hand  the  experiments  of  Eoenig  with  short  sounding  rods,  and 
those  of  Preyer  with  Appunn's  tuning-forks,  have  established  the  fact  that  very 
high  tones  with  from  4000  to  40,000  vibrations  in  a  second  can  be  heard,  but  that 
for  these  the  sensation  of  interval  is  extremely  deficient.  Even  intervals  of  a  Fifth 
or  an  Octave  in  the  highest  positions  are  only  doubtfully  recognised  and  are  often 
wrongly  appreciated  by  practised  musicians.  Even  the  major  Third  c" — e"  [4096  : 
5120  vibrations]  was  at  one  time  heard  as  a  Second,  at  another  as  a  Fourth  or  a 
Fifth  ;  and  at  still  greater  heights  even  Octaves  and  Fifths  were  confused.  ^ 

If  we  maintain  the  hypothesis,  that  every  nervous  fibre  hiaars  in  its  own  peculiar 
pitch,  we  should  have  to  conclude  that  the  vibrating  parts  of  the  ear  which  convey 
these  sensations  of  the  highest  tones  to  the  ear,  are  much  less  sharply  defined  in  their 
capabilities  of  resonance,  than  those  for  deeper  tones.  This  means  that  they  lose  any 
motion  excited  in  them  comparatively  soon,  and  are  also  comparatively  more  easily 
brought  into  the  state  of  motion  necessary  for  sensation.  This  last  assumption 
must  be  made,  because  for  parts  which  are  so  strongly  damped,  the  possibility  of 
adding  together  many  separate  impulses  is  very  hmited,  and  the  construction  of  the 
auditory  ciliae  in  the  Uttle  bags  of  the  otohths  seems  to  me  more  suited  for  this 
purpose  than  that  of  the  shortest  fibres  of  the  basilar  membrane.  If  this  hypo- 
thesis is  confirmed  we  should  have  to  regard  the  auditory  ciliae  as  the  bearers  of 
squeaking,  hissing,  chirping,  crackling  sensations  of  sound,  and  to  consider  their 
reaction  as  differing  only  in  degree  from  that  of  the  cochlear  fibres.f 

♦  Pflueger,   Archiv,  fUr  Physiologic,  vol.  f  [See  App.  XX.  seet.  L.  art.  5.— Tmiw- 

^-  ^^r.]  Digitized  by  ^.jOOgie 


PAET    11. 

ON  THE  INTERRUPTIONS  OF  HARMONY. 


COMBINATIONAL  TONES  AND  BEATS/ 
CONSONANCE  AND  DISSONANCE. 


CHAPTER  VII. 

COMBIKATIONAD  TONBS. 

In  the  first  part  of  this  book  we  had  to  enunciate  and  constantly  apply  the  pro- 
position that  oscillatory  motions  of  the  air  and  other  elastic  bodies,  produced  by 
several  sources  of  sound  acting  simultaneously,  are  always  the  exact  sum  of  the 
individual  motions  producible  by  each  source  separately.  This  law  is  of  extreme 
importance  in  the  theory  of  sound,  because  it  reduces  the  consideration  of  com- 
pound cases  to  those  of  simple  ones.    But  it  must  be  observed  that  this  law  holds 

^  strictly  only  in  the  case  where  the  vibrations  in  all  parts  of  the  mass  of  air  and  of 
the  sonorous  elastic  bodies  are  of  infinitesimally  small  dimensions  ;  that  is  to  say, 
only  when  the  alterations  of  density  of  the  elastic  bodies  are  so  small  that  they, 
may  be  disregarded  in  comparison  with  the  whole  density  of  the  same  body ;  and 
in  the  same  way,  only  when  the  displacements  of  the  vibrating  particles  vanish  as 
compared  with  the  dimensions  of  the  whole  elastic  body.  Now  certainly  in  all 
practical  applications  of  this  law  to  sonorous  bodies,  the  vibrations  are  always 
very  small,  and  near  enough  to  being  infinitesimally  small  for  this  law  to  hold 
witii  great  exactness  even  for  the  real  sonorous  vibrations  of  musical  tones,  and  by 
far  the  greater  part  of  their  phenomena  can  be  deduced  from  that  law  in  con- 
formity with  observation.  Still,  however,  there  are  certain  phenomena  which 
result  from  the  fact  that  this  law  does  not  hold  with  perfect  exactness  for  vibra- 
tions of  elastic  bodies,  which,  though  almost  always  very  STnall,  are  far  from  being 
infinitesimally  small.f  One  of  these  phenomena,  with  which  we  are  here  interested, 

IT  is  the  occurrence  of  Combinational  Tones,  which  were  first  discovered  in  1745  by 
Sorge,t  a  German  organist,  and  were  afterwards  generally  known,  although  their 
pitch  was  often  wrongly  assigned,  through  the  Italian  violinist  Tartini  (1754),  from 
whom  they  are  often  called  Tartini' s  tones.  § 

These  tones  are  heard  whenever  two  musical  tones  of  different  pitches  are 

*  [So  much  attention  has  recently  been  views,  before  taking  up  the  Appendix. — Trans- 

paid  to  the  whole  subject  of  this  second  part  lator,] 

—Combinational    Tones   and    Beats— mostly  f  Helmholtz,  on  'Combinational  Tones,' 

since    the    publication  of    the   4th    German  in  Poggendorff's  Annalefit  vol.  xcix.  p.  497. 

edition,  that  I  have  thought  it  advisable  to  Monatsberichte  of  the  Berlin  Academy,  May  22, 

give  a  brief  account  of  the  investigations  of  1856.    From  this  last  an  extract  is  given  in 

Koenig,  Bosanquet,  and  Preyer  in  App.  XX.  Appendix  XII. 

sect.  L.,  and  merely  add  a  few  footnotes   to  t  Vorgemach   musikalischer    Composilion 

refer  the  reader  to  them  where  they  especially  (Antechamber  of  musical  composition), 

relate  to  the  statements  in  the  text.    But  the  §  [In  England  they  have  hence  been  often 

reader  should  study  the  text  of  this  second  called  by  Tartini's  name,  ierzi  suoni,  or  third 

part,  so  as  to  be  familiar  with  Prof.  HelmhoUz's  sounds,  resulting  from  the  combination  of  two. 


Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


CHAP.  VII.  COMBINATIONAL  TONES.  IS3 

sounded  together,  loudly  and  continuously.  The  pitch  of  a  combinational  tone 
is  generally  different  from  that  of  either  of  the  generating  tones,  or  of  their 
harmonic  upper  partials.  In  experiments,  the  combinational  are  readily  distin- 
guished &om  the  upper  partial  tones,  by  not  being  heard  when  only  one  generating 
tone  is  sounded,  and  by  appearing  simultaneously  with  the  second  tone.  Combi- 
national tones  are  of  two  kinds.  The  first  class,  discovered  by  Sorge  and  Tartini, 
I  have  termed  differential  tones,  because  their  pitch  number  is  the  difference  of 
the  pitch  numbers  of  the  generating  tones.  The  second  class  of  summational 
tones,  having  their  pitch  number  equal  to  the  sum  of  the  pitch  numbers  of  the 
generating  tones,  were  discovered  by  myself. 

On  investigaidng  the  combinational  tones  of  two  compound  musical  tones,  we 
find  that  both  the  primary  and  the  upper  partial  tones  may  give  rise  to  both  dif- 
ferential and  summational  tones.  In  such  cases  the  number  of  combinational 
tones  is  very  great.  But  it  must  be  observed  that  generally  the  differential  are  H 
stronger  than  the  summational  tones,  and  that  the  stronger  generating  simple 
tones  also  produce  the  stronger  combinational  tones.  The  combinational  tones, 
indeed,  increase  in  a  much  greater  ratio  than  the  generating  tones,  and  diminish 
also  more  rapidly.  Now  since  in  musical  compound  tones  the  prime  generally  pre- 
dominates over  the  partials,  the  differential  tones  of  the  two  primes  are  generally 
heard  more  loudly  than  all  the  rest,  and  were  consequently  first  discovered.  They 
are  most  easily  heard  when  the  two  generating  tones  are  less  than  an  octave  apart, 
because  in  that  case  the  differential  is  deeper  than  either  of  the  two  generating 
tones.  To  hear  it  at  first,  choose  two  tones  which  can  be  held  with  great  force  for 
some  time,  and  form  a  justly  intoned  harmonic  interval.  First  sound  the  low 
tone  and  then  the  high  one.  On  properly  directing  attention,  a  weaker  low  tone 
will  be  heard  at  the  moment  that  the  higher  note  is  struck ;  this  is  the  required 
combinational  tone.*  For  particular  instruments,  as  the  harmonium,  the  com- 
binational tones  can  be  made  more  audible  by  properly  tuned  resonators.  In  this  ^ 
case  the  tones  are  generated  in  the  air  contained  within  the  instrument.  But  in 
other  cases,  where  they  are  generated  solely  within  the  ear,  the  resonators  are  of 
little  or  no  use. 

A  commoner  English  name  is  ^mv«  ^rmontcj,  The  differential  tones  are  well  heard  on  the 
which  is  inapplicable,  as  they  are  not  neces-  English  concertina,  for  the  same  reason  as  on 
sarily  graver  than  both  of  the  generating  tones.  the  harmonium.  High  notes  forming  Semi- 
Prof.  Tyndall  calls  them  resultant  Umes,  I  tones  tell  well.  It  is  convenient  to  choose 
prefer  retaining  the  Latin  expression,  first  in-  close  dissonant  intervals  for  first  examples  in 
troduced,  as  Prof.  Preyer  informs  as  (Akiiati-  order  to  dissipate  the  old  notion  that  the 
scJie  Untersuchungen,^.  II) J  hy  Gt.XJ.  A,Yieih  'grave  harmonic'  is  necessarily  the  *true 
(d.  1836  in  Dessau)  in  Gilbert's  Annalen  der  fundamental  bass'  of  the  'chord.'  It  is  very 
Physik  1805,  vol.  xxi.  p.  265,  but  only  for  the  easy  when  playing  two  high  generating  notes, 
tones  here  distinguished  as  differential,  and  as  g"'  and  g"%  or  the  last  and  a'",  to  hear  at 
afterwards  used  by  Scheibler  and  Prof.  Helm-  the  same  time  the  rattle  of  the  beats  (see  next 
holtz.  I  shall,  however,  use  '  combinational  chapter)  and  the  deep  combinational  tones 
tones '  to  express  all  the  additional  tones  which  about  F^Z  and  0,%  ,  much  resembling  a  thrash- 
are  heard  when  two  notes  are  sounded  at  the  ing  machine  two  or  three  fields  off.  The  beats  % 
same  time. — Translator,']  and  the  differentials  have  the  same  frequency 
*  [I  have  found  that  combinational  tones  (note  p.  i  id).  See  infr^,  App.  XX.  sect.  L.  art. 
can  be  made  quite  audible  to  a  hundred  people  5,  /.  The  experiment  can  also  be  made  with 
at  once,  by  means  of  two  flageolet  fifes  or  6''  cT'  and  h"\>  h''  on  any  harmonium.  And  if 
whistles,  blown  as  strongly  as  possible.  I  all  three  notes  \>'\> ,  6'',  <i"  are  held  down  to- 
choose  very  closedissonant  intervals  because  the  gether,  the  ear  can  perceive  the  two  sets  of 
great  depth  of  the  low  tone  is  much  more  strik-  beats  of  the  upper  notes  as  sharp  high  rattles, 
ing,  being  very  far  below  anything  that  can  be  and  the  beats  of  the  two  combinational  tones, 
touched  by  the  instrument  itself.  Thus  ^"'  about  the  pitch  of  C,  which  have  altogether  a 
being  sounded  loudly  on  one  fife  by  an  assis-  different  character  and  frequency.  On  the 
iant,  I  give/"'S  ,  when  a  deep  note  is  instantly  Harmouical,  notes  h"  d'  should  beat  66,  notes 
heard  which,  if  the  interval  were  pure,  would  6  "b  h"  should  beat  39*6,  and  notes  ''h''\>  6'"b 
be  ^,  and  is  sufficiently  near  to  g  to  be  recog-  should  beat  26*4  in  a  second,  and  these  should 
nised  as  extremely  deep.  As  a  second  experi-  be  the  pitches  of  their  combinational  notes  ; 
ment,  the  &'"  being  held  as  before,  I  give  first  the  two  first  should  therefore  beat  26*4  times 
f'"%  and  men  e""  in  succession.  If  the  inter-  in  a  second,  and  the  two  last  13*2  times  in  a 
vals  were  pure  the  combinational  tones  would  second.  But  the  tone  26'4  is  so  difficult  to 
jump  from  g  to  c\  and  in  reality,  the  jump  is  hear  that  the  beats  are  not  distinct.— Trans- 
very  nearly  the  same  and  quite  appreciable.  latorJ] 

Digitized  by  VjOOQIC 


'54 


COMBINATIONAL  TONES. 


PABT  II. 


The  following  table  gives  the  first  differential  tones  of  the  usual  harmonic 
intervals : — 


Interrals 

Ratio  of  the 

yibrational 

numbers 

Difference  of 
the  same 

The  combinational  tone  is  deeper  than 

Octave     . 
Fifth       . 
Fourth     . 
Major  Third 
Minor  Third 
Major  Sixth    . 
Minor  Sixth    . 

1 : 2 
2:3 

3:4 
4:5 
5:6 
3:5 
5:8 

2 

3 

a  Unison 

an  Octave 

a  Twelfth 

Two  Octaves 

Two  Octaves  and  a  major  Third 

a  Fifth 

a  major  Sixth 

or  in  ordinary  musical  notation,  the  generating  tones  being  written  as  minims  and 
5f  the  diHerential  tones  as  crotchets — 

Octave.      Fifth.      Fourth.      Major       Minor       Major       Minor 

Third.       Third.       Sixth.       Sixth. 


t 


^. 


3a: 


^ 


IZS? 


m 


^ 


Wheu  the  ear  has  learned  to  hear  the  combinational  tones  of  pure  intervals 
and  sustained  tones,  it  will  be  able  to  hear  them  from  inharmonic  intervals  and  in 
the  rapidly  fading  notes  of  a  pianoforte.  The  combinational  tones  from  inhar- 
^  monic  intervals  are  more  difficult  to  hear,  because  these  intervals  beat  more  or  less 
strongly,  as  we  shaH  have  to  explain  hereafter.  The  combinational  tones  arising 
from  such  as  fade  rapidly,  for  example  those  of  the  pianoforte,  are  not  strong 
enough  to  be  heard  except  at  the  first  instant,  and  die  off  sooner  than  the  gene- 
rating tones.  Combinational  tones  are  also  in  general  easier  to  hear  &om  the  simple 
tones  of  tuning-forks  and  stopped  organ  pipes  than  from  compound  tones  where  a 
number  of  other  secondary  tones  are  also  present.  These  compound  tones,  as  has 
been  already  said,  also  generate  a  number  of  differential  tones  by  their  harmonic 
upper  partials,  and  these  easily  distract  attention  from  the  differential  tones  of  the 
primes.  Combinational  tones  of  this  kind,  arising  from  the  upper  partials,  are 
frequently  heard  from  the  violin  and  harmonium. 

Example, — Take  the  major  Third  c'e',  ratio  of  pitch  numbers  4  :  5.  First  difference  i,  that 
is  C.  The  first  harmonic  upper  partial  of  c'  is  c'',  relative  pitch  number  8.  Ratio  of  this  and 
e',  S  :  8,  difference  3,  that  is  g.  The  first  upper  partial  of  e'  is  6",  relative  pitch  number  10 ; 
m  ratio  for  this  and  c',  4  :  10,  difference  6,  that  is  g'.  Then  again  c"  e"  have  ratio  8  :  10,  difference 
2,  that  is  c.  Heoce,  taking  only  the  first  upper  partials  we  have  the  series  of  combinational 
tones  I,  Si  6,  z  01  Ct  g^g't  c.    Of  these  the  tone  3,  or  ^,  is  often  easily  perceived. 

These  multiple  combinational  tones  cannot  in  general  be  distinctly  heard,  except 
when  the  generating  compound  tones  contain  audible  harmonic  upper  partials. 
Yet  we  cannot  assert  that  the  combinational  tones  are  absent,  where  such  partials 
are  absent ;  but  in  that  case  they  are  so  weak  that  the  ear  does  not  readily  recognise 
them  beside  the  loud  generating  tones  and  first  differential.  In  the  first  place 
theory  leads  us  to  conclude  that  they  do  exist  in  a  weak  state,  and  in  the  next 
place  the  beats  of  impure  intervals,  to  be  discussed  presently,  also  establish  their 
existence.  In  this  case  we  may,  as  Hallstroem  suggests,*  consider  the  multiple 
combinational  tones  to  arise  thus  :  the  first  differential  tone,  or  combinational  tone 
of  the  first  order,  by  combination  with  the  generating  tones  themselves,  produce 
other  differential  tones,  or  combinational  tones  of  the  second  order ;  these  again 

*  Poggendorff's  X/i;i^/c;i,  vol.  xxiv.p.  438. 

Digitized  by  VjOOQIC 


GHAP.  VII. 


COMBINATIONAL  TONES. 


155 


produce  new  ones  with  the  generators  and  differentials  of  the  first  order,  and 
so  on. 

Example, — Take  two  simple  tones  &  and  e\  ratio  4:5,  difference  i,  differential  tone  of  the 
first  order  C.  This  with  the  generators  gives  the  ratios  i  :  4  and  I  :  5,  differences  3  and  4, 
differential  tones  of  the  second  order  g,  and  c'  once  more.  The  new  tone  3,  gives  with  the 
generators  the  ratios  3  :  4  and  3:5,  differences  i  and  2,  giving  the  differential  tones  of  the  third 
order  G  and  c,  and  the  same  tone  3  gives  with  the  differential  of  the  first  order  i,  the  ratio  1:3, 
difference  2,  and  hence  as  a  differential  of  the  fourth  order  c  once  more,  and  so  on.  The  dif- 
ferential tones  of  different  orders  which  coincide  when  the  interval  is  perfect,  as  it  is  supposed  to 
be  in  this  example,  no  longer  exactly  coincide  when  the  generating  interval  is  not  pure ;  and 
consequently  such  beats  are  heard,  as  would  result  from  the  presence  of  these  tones.  More  on 
this  hereafter. 

The  differential  tones  of  different  orders  for  different  intervals  are  given  in  the 
following  notes,  where  the  generators  are  minims,  the  combinational  tones  of  the  ^ 
first  order  crotchets,  of  the  second  quavers,  and  so  on.     The  same  tones  also  occur 
with  compound  generators  as  combinational  tones  of  their  upper  paartials.* 


Octave. 

J 


Fifth. 


Fourth. 


Major  Third. 


=}= 


-A^ 


3t: 


-<s»- 


-«s>- 


r 


■Wr 


-^ 


:J^«e 


Minor  Third. 


Major  Sixth. 


Minor  Sixth. 


The  series  are  broken  off  as  soon  as  the  last  order  of  differentials  furnishes  no 
firesh  tones.  In  general  these  examples  shew  that  the  complete  series  of  harmonic 
partial  tones  i,  2,  3,  4,  5,  &c.,  up  to  the  generators  tliemselves,t  is  produced. 

The  second  kind  of  combinational  tones,  which  I  have  distinguished  Bkastimma- 
tional,  is  generally  much  weaker  in  sound  than  the  first,  and  is  only  to  be  heard 


♦  [These  examples  are  best  calculated  by 
giving  to  the  notes  in  the  example  the  numbers 
representing  the  harmonics  on  p.  22c.    Thus 

Octave,  notes  4  :  8.    Diff.  8  —  4  =  4. 

Fifth,  notes  4  :  6.    Diff.  6-4  =  2. 
2nd  order,  4  —  2  =  2,  6  —  2  =  4, 

Fourth,  notes  6  :  8.    Diff.  8-6  =  2. 
2nd  order,  8  —  2  =  6,6  —  2  =  4. 
3rd  order,  6—4  =  2,  6-2  =  4. 

Major  Third,  notes  4  :  5.     Diff.  5—4  =  1. 
2nd.  4-1=3.  S->=4. 
3rd.  4-3  =  ii  5-3  =  2. 
4th.   4-2  =  2,4-1=3. 

Minor  Third,  notes  5  :  6.     DifiF.  6-5  -i. 
2nd.  5  — 1=4,  6-1  =  5. 
3rd.    5-4=1,6-4-2. 
4th.   4-1=3,6-2-4. 
5th.   6-4-2.  6-33. 


Major  Sixth,  notes  6  :  10.    Diff.  10—6  =  4.  -vi 
2nd.  10-4  =  6,6-4  =  2.  ^' 

3rd.   10-2  =  8,  6-2 « 4. 
4th.    6-4  =  2. 

Minor  Sixth,  notes  5  :  8.  Diff.  8-5  =  3. 
2nd.  5-3  =  2,8-3  =  5- 
3rd.  5-2  =  3,8-2  =  6. 
4th.  3-2=1,5-3  =  2. 
5th.  5-1=4.8-1=7. 
6th.   8-7  =  5-4=1,4-2  =  2,8-4  =  4. 

The  existence  of  these  differential  tones  of 
higher  orders  cannot  be  considered  as  com- 
pletely established. — Translator,'] 

,  t  [See  App.  XX.  sect.  L.  art.  7,  for  the 
influence  of  such  a  series  on  the  consonance  of 
simple  tones.  It  is  not  to  be  supposed  that  all 
these  tones  are  audible.  Mr.  Bosanquet  derives 
them  direct  from  the  generators,  see  App.  XX. 
sect.  L.  art.  5,  a, —  Translator.] 


Digitized  by 


\^oogle 


156 


COMBINATIONAL  TONES. 


PABT  n. 


with  decent  ease  under  peculiarly  favourable  circumstances  on  the  harmonium  and 
polyphonic  siren.  Scarcely  any  but  the  first  summational  tone  can  be  perceived, 
having  a  vibrational  number  equal  to  the  sum  of  those  of  the  generators.  Of  course 
sunmiational  tones  may  also  arise  from  the  harmonic  upper  partials.  Since  their 
vibrational  number  is  always  equal  to  the  sum  of  the  other  two,  they  are  always 
higher  in  pitch  than  either  of  the  two  generators.  The  following  notes  will  shew 
their  nature  for  the  simple  intervals : — 


In  relation  to  music  I  will  here  remark  at  once  that  many  of  these  summa- 
tional tones  form  extremely  inharmonic  intervals  with  the  generators.  Were  they 
not  generally  so  weak  on  most  instruments,  they  would  give  rise  to  intoler- 
able dissonances.  In  reality,  the  major  and  minor  Third,  and  the  minor  Sixth, 
sound  very  badly  indeed  on  the  polyphonic  siren,  where  all  combinational  tones 
are  remarkably  loud,  whereas  the  Octave,  Fifth,  and  major  Sixth  are  very  beautiful. 
Even  the  Fourth  on  this  siren  has  only  the  ejQfect  of  a  tolerably  harmonious  chord 
of  the  minor  Seventh. 

^  It  was  formerly  believed  that  the  combinational  tones  were  purely  subjective, 
and  were  produced  in  the  ear  itself .f  Differential  tones  alone  were  known,  and  these 
were  connected  with  the  beats  which  usually  result  from  the  simultaneous  sounding 
of  two  tones  of  nearly  the  same  pitch,  a  phenomenon  to  be  considered  in  the  follow- 
ing chapters.  It  was  believed  that  when  these  beats  occurred  with  sufficient 
rapidity,  the  individual  increments  of  loudness  might  produce  the  sensation  of  a 
new  tone,  just  as  numerous  ordinary  impulses  of  the  air  would,  and  that  the 
frequency  of  such  a  tone  would  be  equal  to  the  frequency  of  the  beats.  But  this 
supposition,  in  the  first  place,  does  not  explain  the  origin  of  summational  tones, 
being  confined  to  the  differentials  ;  secondly,  it  may  be  proved  that  under  certain 
conditions  the  combinational  tones  exist  objectively,  independently  of  the  ear 
which  would  have  had  to  gather  the  beats  into  a  new  tone ;  and  thirdly,  this 
supposition  cannot  be  reconciled  with  the  law  confirmed  by  all  other  experiments, 
that  the  only  tones  which  the  ear  hears,  correspond  to  pendular  vibrations  of  the 

fair.  J 

And  in  reaUty  a  different  cause  for  the  origin  of  combinational  tones  can  be 
estabUshed,  which  has  already  been  mentioned  in  general  terms  (p.  152c).  When- 
ever the  vibrations  of  the  air  or  of  other  elastic  bodies  which  are  set  in  motion  at 
the  same  time  by  two  generating  simple  tones,  are  so  powerful  that  they  can  no  longer 
be  considered  infinitely  small,  mathematical  theory  shews  that  vibrations  of  the 
air  must  arise  which  have  the  same  frequency  as  the  combinational  tones.§ 

Particular  instruments  give  very  powerful  combinational  tones.     The  condition 


*  [The  notation  of  the  last  5  bars  has  been 
altered  to  agree  with  the  diagram  of  harmonicB 
of  C  on  p.  22c.^  Translator.] 

f  [The  result  of  Mr.  Bosanqnet's  and  Prof. 
Preyer's  quite  recent  experiments  is  to  shew 
that  tliey  are  so.  See  App.  XX.  sect.  L.  art.  4, 
fc,  c.  —  Translator,] 

X  [For  Prof.  Preyer's  remarks   on    these 


objections,  and  for  other  objections,  see  App. 
XX.  sect.  Ii.  art.  5,  6,  c  — Translator.] 

§  [The  tones  supposed  to  arise  from  beats, 
and  the  differential  tones  thus  generated,  are 
essentially  distinct,  having  sometimes  the  same 
but  frequently  different  pitch  numbers.  See 
App.  XX.  sect.  L.  art.  3,  d, — Translator,] 


Digitized  by 


Google 


CHAP.vn.  COMBINATIONAL  TONES.  157 

for  their  generation  is  that  the  same  mass  of  air  should  be  violently  agitated  by 
two  simple  tones  simultaneously.  This  takes  place  most  powerfully  in  the  poly- 
phonic siren/  in  which  the  same  rotating  disc  contains  two  or  more  series  of 
holes  which  are  blown  upon  simultaneously  from  the  same  windchest.  The  air 
of  the  windchest  is  condensed  whenever  the  holes  are  closed  ;  on  the  holes  being 
opened,  a  large  quantity  of  air  escapes,  and  the  pressure  is  considerably  diminished. 
Consequently  the  air  in  the  windchest,  and  partly  even  that  in  the  bellows,  as 
can  be  easily  felt,  comes  into  violent  vibration.  If  two  rows  of  holes  are  blown, 
vibrations  arise  in  the  air  of  the  windchest  corresponding  to  both  tones,  and  each 
row  of  openings  gives  vent  not  to  a  stream  of  air  uniformly  supplied,  but  to  a 
stream  of  air  already  set  in  vibration  by  the  other  tone.  Under  these  circumstances 
the  combinational  tones  are  extremely  powerful,  almost  as  powerful,  indeed,  as  the 
generators.  Their  objective  existence  in  the  mass  of  air  can  be  proved  by  vibra- 
ting membranes  tuned  to  be  in  unison  with  the  combinational  tones.  Such  f 
membranes  are  set  in  sympathetic  vibration  immediately  upon  both  generating 
tones  being  sounded  simultaneously,  but  remain  at  rest  if  only  one  or  the  other  of 
them  is  sounded.  Indeed,  in  this  case  the  summational  tones  are  so  powerful 
that  they  make  all  chords  extremely  unpleasant  which  contain  Thirds  or  minor 
Sixths.  Instead  of  membranes  it  is  more  convenient  to  use  the  resonators  already 
reconmiended  for  investigating  harmonic  upper  partial  tones.  Besonators  are 
also  unable  to  reinforce  a  tone  when  no  pendular  vibrations  actually  exist  in  the 
air ;  they  have  no  effect  on  a  tone  which  exists  only  in  auditory  sensation,  and 
hence  they  can  be  used  to  discover  whether  a  combinational  tone  is  objectively 
present.  They  are  much  more  sensitive  than  membranes,  and  are  well  adapted 
for  the  clear  recognition  of  very  weak  objective  tones. 

The  conditions  in  the  harmonium  are  similar  to  those  in  the  siren.  Here,  too, 
there  is  a  common  windchest,  and  when  two  keys  are  pressed  down,  we  have  two 
openings  which  are  closed  and  opened  rhythmically  by  the  tongues.  In  this  case  f 
also  the  air  in  the  common  receptacle  is  violently  agitated  by  both  tones,  and  aii- 
is  blown  through  each  opening  which  has  been  already  set  in  vibration  by  the 
other  tongue.  Hence  in  this  instrument  also  the  combinational  tones  are  objectively 
present,  and  comparatively  very  distinct,  but  they  are  far  from  being  as  powerful 
as  on  the  siren,  probably  because  the  windchest  is  very  much  larger  in  proportion 
to  the  openings,  and  hence  the  air  which  escapes  during  the  short  opening  of  an 
exit  by  the  oscillating  tongue  cannot  be  sufBcient  to  diminish  the  pressui-e  sensibly. 
In  the  harmonium  also  the  combinational  tones  are  very  clearly  reinforced  by 
resonators  tuned  to  be  in  unison  with  them,  especially  the  first  and  second  dif- 
ferential and  the  first  sunmaational  tone.f  Nevertheless  I  have  convinced  myself,  by 
particular  experiments,  that  even  in  this  instrument  the  greater  part  of  the  force 
of  the  combinational  tone  is  generated  in  the  ear  itself.  I  arranged  the  portvents 
in  the  instrument  so  that  one  of  the  two  generators  was  suppUed  with  air  by  the 
bellows  moved  below  by  the  foot,  and  the  second  generator  was  blown  by  tht  ^ 
reserve  bellows,  which  was  first  pumped  full  and  then  cut  off  by  drawing  out  the 
so-called  expression-stop,  and  I  then  found  that  the  combinational  tones  were  not 
much  weaker  than  for  the  usual  arrangement.  But  the  objective  portion  which 
tlie  resonators  reinforce  was  much  weaker.  The  noted  examples  given  above 
(pp.  154-5-6)  will  easily  enable  any  one  to  find  the  digitals  which  must  be 
pressed  down  in  order  to  produce  a  combinational  tone  in  unison  with  a  given 
resonator. 

On  the  other  hand,  when  the  places  in  which  the  two  tones  are  struck  are 
entirely  separate  and  have  no  mechanical  connection,  as,  for  example,  if  they  come 
from  two  singers,  two  separate  wind  instruments,  or  two  violins,  the  reinforcement 

*  A  detailed  description  of  this  instrnment  apparent  reinforcement  by  a  resonator  arose 

will  be  given  in  the  next  chapter.  from  imperfect  blocking  of  both  ears  when 

t  [The  experiments  of  Bosanquet,  App.  XX.  using  it.  See  also  p.  43d',  note,— Translator.] 
sect.  L.  art.  4,  6,  render  it  probable  that  this 

Digitized  by  V^OOQIC 


158  COMBINATIONAL   TONES.  part  11. 

of  the  combinational  tones  by  resonators  is  small  and  dubious.  Here,  then,  there 
does  not  exist  in  the  air  any  clearly  sensible  pendular  vibration  corresponding  to 
the  combinational  tone,  and  we  must  conclude  that  such  tones,  which  are  often 
powerfully  audible,  are  really  produced  in  the  ear  itself.  But  analogously  to  the 
former  cases  we  are  justified  in  assuming  in  this  case  also  that  the  external  vibra- 
ting parts  of  the  ear,  the  drumskin  and  auditory  ossicles,  are  really  set  in  a  suffi- 
ciently powerful  combined  vibration  to  generate  combinational  tones,  so  that  the 
vibrations  which  correspond  to  combinational  tones  may  really  exist  objectively  in 
the  parts  of  the  ear  without  existing  objectively  in  the  external  air.  A  slight  rein- 
forcement of  the  combinational  tone  in  this  case  by  the  corresponding  resonator 
may,  therefore,  arise  from  the  drumskin  of  the  ear  communicating  to  the  air  in  the 
resonator  those  particular  vibrations  which  correspond  to  the  combinational  tone.* 
Now  it  so  happens  that  in  the  construction  of  the  external  parts  of  the  ear  for 

fl  conducting  sound,  there  are  certain  conditions  which  are  peculiarly  favourable  for 
the  generation  of  combinational  tones.  First  we  have  the  unsynmaetrical  form  of 
the  drumskin  itself.  Its  radial  fibres,  which  are  externally  convex,  undergo  a  much 
greater  alteration  of  tension  when  they  make  an  oscillation  of  moderate  amplitude 
towards  the  inside,  than  when  the  oscillation  takes  place  towards  the  outside. 
For  this  purpose  it  is  only  necessary  that  the  amplitude  of  the  oscillation  should 
not  be  too  small  a  fraction  of  the  minute  depth  of  the  arc  made  by  these  radial 
fibres.  Under  these  circumstances  deviations  from  the  simple  superposition  of 
vibrations  arise  for  very  much  smaller  amphtudes  than  is  the  case  when  the  vibra- 
ting body  is  symmetrically  constructed  on  both  sides.f 

But  a  more  important  circumstance,  as  it  seems  to  me,  when  the  tones  are 
powerful,  is  the  loose  formation  of  the  joint  between  the  hammer  and  anvil  (p.  1336). 
If  the  handle  of  the  hammer  is  driven  inwards  by  the  drumskin,  the  anvil  and 
stirrup  must  follow  the  motion  unconditionally.     But  that  is  not  the  case  for  the 

f  subsequent  outward  motion  of  the  handle  of  the  hammer,  during  which  the  tettli 
of  the  two  ossicles  need  not  catch  each  other.  In  this  case  the  ossicles  may  cli^k. 
Now  I  seem  to  hear  this  clicking  in  my  own  ear  whenever  a  very  strong  and  deep 
tone  is  brought  to  bear  upon  it,  even  when,  for  example,  it  is  produced  by  a  tuning- 
fork  held  between  the  fingers,  in  which  there  is  certainly  nothing  that  can  make 
any  click  at  all. 

This  peculiar  feeling  of  mechanical  tingling  in  the  ear  had  long  ago  struck  me 
when  two  clear  and  powerful  soprano  voices  executed  passages  in  Thirds,  in  which 
case  the  combinational  tone  comes  out  very  distinctly.  If  the  phases  of  the  two 
tones  are  so  related  that  after  every  fourth  oscillation  of  the  deeper  and  every  fifth 
of  the  higher  tone,  there  ensues  a  considerable  outward  displacement  of  the  drum- 
skin, sufficient  to  cause  a  momentary  loosening  in  the  joint  between  the  hammer 
and  anvil,  a  series  of  blows  will  be  generated  between  the  two  bones,  which  would 
be  absent  if  the  connection  were  firm  and  the  oscillation  regular,  and  these  blows 

^  taken  together  would  exactly  generate  the  first  differential  tone  of  the  interval  of 
a  major  Third.     Similarly  for  other  intervals. 

It  must  also  be  remarked  that  the  same  peculiarities  in  the  construction  of  a 
sonorous  body  which  makes  it  suitable  for  allowing  combinational  tones  to  be  heard 
when  it  is  excited  by  two  waves  of  diff^erent  pitch,  must  also  cause  a  single  simple 
tone  to  excite  in  it  vibrations  corresponding  to  its  harmonic  upper  partials ;  the 
effiect  being  the  same  as  if  this  tone  then  formed  summational  tones  with  itself. 

This  result  ensues  because  a  simple  periodical  force,  corresponding  to  pendular 
vibrations,  cannot  excite  similar  pendular  vibrations  in  the  elastic  body  on  which 
it  acts,  unless  the  elastic  forces  called  into  action  by  the  displacements  of  the  ex- 

*  [See   latter    half  of    Appendix   XVI. —  are  proportional  to  the  first  pover  of  the  am - 

Translator,]  plitude,  whereas  for   symmetrical  ones  they 

f  See  my  paper  on  combinational  tones  are  proportional  to  only  the  second  power  of 

alreajdy  cited,  and  Appendix  XII.    For  unsym-  this  magnitude,  which  is  very  small  in  both 

metrical  vibrating    bodies    the    disturbances  cases. 

Digitized  by  VjOOQlC 


CHAPS.  VII.  VIII.  INTEBFERENCE  OF  SOUND.  159 

cited  body  from  its  position  of  equilibrium,  are  proportional  to  these  displacements 
themselves.  This  is  always  the  case  so  long  as  these  displacements  are  infinitesimal. 
But  if  the  amplitude  of  the  oscillations  is  great  enough  to  cause  a  sensible  devia- 
tion from  this  proportionality,  then  the  vibrations  of  the  exciting  tone  are  increased 
by  others  which  correspond  to  its  harmonic  upper  partial  tones.  That  such  har- 
monic upper  partials  are  occasionally  heard  when  tuning-forks  are  strongly  ex- 
cited, has  been  already  mentioned  (p.  54^).  I  have  lately  repeated  these  experi- 
ments with  forks  of  a  very  low  pitch.  With  such  a  fork  of  64  vib.  I  could,  by 
means  of  proper  resonators,  hear  up  to  the  fifth  partial.  But  then  the  amplitude 
of  the  vibrations  was  almost  a  centimetre  ['3937  inch].  When  a  sharp-edged 
body,  such  as  the  prong  of  a  tuning-fork,  makes  vibrations  of  such  a  length, 
vortical  motions,  differing  sensibly  from  the  law  of  simple  vibrations,  must  arise 
in  the  surrounding  air.  On  the  other  hand,  as  the  sound  of  the  fork  fades,  these 
upper  partials  vanish  long  before  their  prime,  which  is  itself  only  very  weakly  ^ 
audible.  This  agrees  with  our  hypothesis  that  these  partials  arise  from  disturb- 
ances depending  on  the  size  of  the  amplitude. 

Herr  B.  Eoenig,*  with  a  series  of  forks  having  sliding  weights  by  which  the  pitch 
might  be  gradually  altered,  and  provided  also  with  boxes  giving  a  good  resonance 
and  possessing  powerful  tones,  has  investigated  beats  and  combinational  tones,  and 
found  that  those  combinational  tones  were  most  prominent  which  answered  to  the 
difference  of  one  of  the  tones  from  the  partial  tone  of  the  other  which  was  nearest 
to  it  in  pitch ;  and  in  this  research  partial  tones  as  high  as  the  eighth  were  effec- 
tive (at  least  in  the  number  of  beats). f  He  has  unfortunately  not  stated  how  far  the 
corresponding  upper  partials  were  separately  recognised  by  resonators.  J 

Since  the  human  ear  easily  produces  combinational  tones,  for  which  the  prin- 
cipal causes  lying  in  the  construction  of  that  organ  have  just  been  assigned,  it 
must  also  form  upper  partials  for  powerful  simple  tones,  as  is  the  case  for  tuning- 
forks  and  the  masses  of  air  which  they  excite  in  the  observations  described.  Hence  If 
we  cannot  easily  have  the  sensation  of  a  powerful  simple  tone,  without  having  also 
the  sensation  of  its  harmonic  upper  partials.§ 

The  importance  of  combinational  tones  in  the  construction  of  chords  will  appear 
hereafter.  We  have,  however,  first  to  investigate  a  second  phenomenon  of  the 
simultaneous  sounding  of  two  tones,  the  so-called  heats. 


CHAPTEB  Vm. 

ON   THE  BEATS   OF   SIMPLE   TONES. 


We  now  pass  to  the  consideration  of  other  phenomena  accompanying  the  simul  % 
taneous  sounding  of  two  simple  tones,  in  which,  as  before,  the  motions  of  the  air 
and  of  the  other  co-operating  elastic  bodies  without  and  within  the  ear  may  be  con- 
ceived as  an  undisturbed  coexistence  of  two  systems  of  vibrations  corresponding  to 
the  two  tones,  but  where  the  auditory  sensation  no  longer  corresponds  to  the  sum 
of  the  two  sensations  which  the  tones  would  excite  singly.  Beats,  which  have 
now  to  be  considered,  are  essentially  distinguished  from  combinational  tones  as 
follows: — In  combinational  tones  the  composition  of  vibrations  in  the  elastic 
vibrating  bodies  which  are  either  within  or  without  the  ear,  undergoes  certain  dis- 
turbances, although  the  ear  resolves  the  motion  which  is  finally  conducted  to  it, 

*  PoggendorfF's  AnnaLy  vol.  clvii.  pp.  177-  sect.  L. — Translator,] 
236.  X  [Koenig  states   that  no  upper  partials 

f  [Even  with  this  parenthetical  correction,  could  be  heard.     See  Appendix  XX.  sect.  L. 

the  above  is  calculated  to  give  an  inadequate  art.  2,  a,--Translator.] 

impression  of  the  results  of  Koenig's  paper,  §  [See  App.  XX.  sect.  L.  art.  i,ii. — Trans- 

which  is  more  fully  described  in  Appendix  XX.  lator,'\ 

Digitized  by  VjOOQIC 


i6o 


INTERFERENCE   OF  SOUND. 


PART  n. 


into  a  series  of  simple  tones,  according  to  the  usual  law.  In  beats,  on  the 
contrary,  the  objective  motions  of  the  elastic  bodies  follow  the  simple  law ;  but 
the  composition  of  the  sensations  is  disturbed.  As  long  as  several  simple  tones  of 
a  sufiQciently  dijQferent  pitch  enter  the  ear  together,  the  sensation  due  to  each 
remains  undisturbed  in  the  ear,  probably  because  entirely  different  bundles  of 
nerve  fibres  are  affected.    But  tones  of  the  same,  or  of  nearly  the  same  pitch, 

'  which  therefore  affect  the  same  nerve  fibres,  do  not  produce  a  sensation  which  is 
the  sum  of  the  two  they  would  have  separately  excited,  but  new  and  peculiar 
phenomena  aarise  which  we  term  interference^  when  caused  by  two  perfectly  equal 
simple  tones,  and  heats  when  due  to  two  nearly  equal  simple  tones. 

We  will  begin  with  interference.  Suppose  that  a  point  in  the  air  or  ear 
is  set  in  motion  by  some  sonorous  force,  and  that  its  motion  is  represented  by 
the  curve  i,  fig.  53.  Let 

IT  the  second    motion    be  ^'®'  53- 

precisely  the  same  at  the 
same  time  and  be  repre- 
sented by  the  curve  2,  so 
that  the  crests  of  2  fall 
on  the  crests  of  i,  and 
also  the  troughs  of  2  on 
the  troughs  of  i .  If  both 
motions  proceed  at  once, 
the  whole  motion  will  be 
their  sum,  represented  by 
3,  a  curve  of  the  same 
kind  but  with  crests  twice  as  high  and  troughs  twice  as  deep  as  those  of  either  of 
the  others.     Since  the  intensity  of  sound  is  proportional  to  the  square  of  the 

^  amplitude,  we  have  consequently  a  tone  not  of  twice  but  of  four  times  the  loudness 
of  either  of  the  others. 

But  now  suppose  the  vibrations  of  the  second  motion  to  be  displaced  by 


The  curves  to  be  added  will  stand  under  one  another,  as 

Pio.  54. 


half  the  periodic  time. 
4  and  5  in  fig.  54,  and 
when  we  come  to  add 
to  them,  the  heights  of 
the  second  curve  will  be 
still  the  same  as  those 
of  the  first,  but,  being 
always  in  the  contrary 
direction,  the  two  will 
mutually  destroy  each 
other,  giving  as  their 
^  sum  the  straight  line  6,  or  no  vibration  at  all.  In  this  case  the  crests  of  4  are 
added  to  the  troughs  of  5,  and  conversely,  so  that  the  crests  fill  up  the  troughs, 
and  crests  and  troughs  mutually  annihilate  each  other.  The  intensity  of  sound 
also  becomes  nothing,  and  when  motions  are  thus  cancelled  within  the  ear,  sensa- 
tion also  ceases  ;  and  although  each  single  motion  acting  alone  would  excite  the 
corresponding  auditory  sensation,  when  both  act  together  there  is  no  auditory 
sensation  at  all.  One  sound  in  this  case  completely  cancels  what  appears  to  be 
an  equal  sound.  This  seems  extraordinarily  paradoxical  to  ordinary  contempla- 
tion because  our  natural  consciousness  apprehends  sound,  not  as  the  motion  of 
particles  of  the  air,  but  as  something  really  existing  and  analogous  to  the  sensation 
of  sound.  Now  as  the  sensation  of  a  simple  tone  of  the  same  pitch  shews  no  oppo- 
sitions of  positive  and  negative,  it  naturally  appears  impossible  for  one  positive 
sensation  to  cancel  another.  But  the  really  cancelling  things  in  such  a  case-  are 
the  vibrational  impulses  which  the  two  sources  of  sound  exert  on  the  ear.  When 
it  so  happens  that  the  vibrational  impulses  due  to  one  source  constantly  coincide 

Digitized  by  V^OOQIC 


CHAP.  VIII.  INTEKFEEENCE   OP  SOUND.  i6i 

with  opposite  ones  due  to  the  other,  and  exactly  counterbalance  each  other,  no 
motion  can  possibly  ensue  in  the  ear,  and  hence  the  auditory  nerve  can  experience 
no  sensation. 

The  following  are  some  instances  of  sound  cancelling  sound : — 
Put  two  perfectly  similar  stopped  organ  pipes  timed  to  the  same  pitch  close 
beside  each  other  on  the  same  portvent.  Each  one  blown  separately  gives  a 
powerful  tone ;  but  when  they  are  blown  together,  the  motion  of  the  air  in  the 
two  pipes  takes  place  in  such  a  manner  that  as  the  air  streams  out  of  one  it  streams 
into  the  other,  and  hence  an  observer  at  a  distance  hears  no  tone,  but  at  most  the 
rushing  of  the  air.  On  bringing  the  fibre  of  a  feather  near  to  the  lips  of  the 
pipes,  this  fibre  will  vibrate  in  the  same  way  as  if  each  pipe  were  blown  separately. 
Also  if  a  tube  be  conducted  from  the  ear  to  the  mouth  of  one  of  the  pipes,  the 
tone  of  that  pipe  is  heard  so  much  more  powerfully  that  it  cannot  be  entirely 
destroyed  by  the  tone  of  the  other.*  ^ 

Every  tuning-fork  also  exhibits  phenomena  of  interference,  because  the  prongs 
move  in  opposite  directions.  On  striking  a  tuning-fork  and  slowly  revolving  it 
about  its  longitudinal  axis  close  to  the  ear,  it  will  be  found  that  there  are  four 
positions  in  which  the  tone  is  heard  clearly ;  and  four  intermediate  positions  in 
which  it  is  inaudible.  The  four  positions  of  strong  sound  are  those  in  which 
either  one  of  the  prongs,  or  one  of  the  side  surfEkces  of  the  fork,  is  turned  towards 
the  ear.  The  positions  of  no  sound  lie  between  the  former,  almost  in  planes 
which  make  an  angle  of  45^  with  the  sur£a.ces  of  the  prongs,  and  pass  through 
the  axis  of  the  fork.  If  in  fig.  55,  a  and  b  are  the  ends  of  the  fork  seen  from 
above,  c,  d,  e,  f  will  be  the  four  places  of  strong  sound,  and  the  dotted  lines 

the  places  four  of  silence.    The  arrows  under  a 

Fio-  55-  and  b  shew  the  mutual  motion  of  the  two  prongs. 

.  ,  Hence  while  the  prong  a  gives  the  air  about  c  an  im- 

I  ^^''    pulse  in  the  direction  c  a,  the  prong  b  gives  it  an  ^ 

V.  .'         opposite  one.    Both  impulses  only  partially  cancel 

J  . '  one  another  at  c,  because  a  acts  more  powerfully 

thanb.   But  the  dotted  lines  shew  the  places  where 

the  opposite  impulses  from  a  and  b  are  equally 

J  • — ^         ,steong,  and  consequently  completely  cancel  each 

■     >-         -*r —  other.    If  the  ear  be  brought  into  one  of  these 

'\^  places  of  silence  and  a  narrow  tube  be  slipped  over 

one  of  the  prongs  a  or  b,  taking  care  not  to  touch  it, 

/'  '\,  the  sound  will  be  immediately  augmented,  because 

/  \       the  influence  of  the  covered  prong  is  almost  entirely 

''  '^     destroyed,  and  the  uncovered  prong  therefore  acts 

alone  and  undisturbed.f 
A  double  siren  which  I  have  had  constructed  is  very  convenient  for  the  demon- 
stration of  these  relations.^  Fig.  56  (p.  162)  is  a  perspective  view  of  this  instru-  fl 
ment.  It  is  composed  of  two  of  Dove's  polyphonic  sirens,  of  the  kind  previously 
mentioned,  p.  13a ;  ao  and  ai  are  the  two  windchests,  Cq  and  c^  the  discs  attached 
to  a  common  axis,  on  which  a  screw  is  introduced  at  k,  to  drive  a  counting 
apparatus  which  can  be  introduced,  as  described  on  p.  12b,  The  upper  box  a^ 
can  be  turned  round  its  axis,  by  means  of  a  toothed  wheel,  in  which  works  a 
smaller  wheel  e  provided  with  the  driving  handle  d  §.  The  axis  of  the  box  a^ 
round  which  it  turns,  is  a  prolongation  of  the  upper  pipe  g,,  which  conducts 
the  wind.    On  each  of  the  two  discs  of  the  siren  are  four  rows  of  holes,  which 

*   [If  a  screen  of  any  sort,  as  the  hand,  be  resonance  chamber,  the  alternation  of  sound 

interposed^  between  the  mouths  of  the  pipe,  and  silence,  Ac,  can  be  made  audible  to  many 

the  tone  is  immediately  restored,  and   then  persons  at  once. — Translator^] 
generally  remains  even  if  the  hand  be  re-  J  Constructed  by  the  mechanician  Sauer- 

moved. — Translator.]  wald  in  Berlin. 

t  [If  instead  of  bringing  the  tuning-fork  to  §  [Three  turns  of  the  handle  cause  one 

the  ear,  it  be  slowly  turned  before  a  proper  turn  of  the  box  round  its  axis.— Translator .] 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQ IC 


l62 


INTERFERENCE  OP  SOUND. 


PABT  n« 


can  be  either  blown  separately  or  together  in  any  combination  at  pleasure,  and  at  i 
are  the  studs  for  opening  and  closing  the  series  of  holes  by  a  peculiar  arrange- 
ment.* The  lower  disc  has  four  rows  of  8,  lo,  12,  1 8  holes,  the  upper  of  9,  12, 
15,  16.    Hence  if  we  call  the  tone  of  8  holes  c,  the  lower  disc  gives  the  tones  c,  e, 

Pio.  56. 


gf,  6/  and  the  upper  d,  gr,  b,  d.    We  are  therefore  able  to  produce  the  following 
intervals : — 

1.  Unison  :  gg  on  the  two  discs  simultaneously. 

2.  Octaves  :  c  d  and  dd!  on  tlie  two. 


*  Described  in  Appendn  XIII. 


Digitized  by  VjOOQIC 


CHAP.7III.  INTERFERENCE  OP  SOUND.  163 

3.  Fifths !  c  g  and  g  dl  either  on  the  lower  disc  alone  or  on  both  discs  together. 

4.  Fourths :  d  g  and  g  c'  on  the  upper  disc  alone  or  on  both  together. 

5.  Major  Third :  c  a  on  the  lower  alone,  and  ^  6  on  the  upper  alone,  or  gh  on 
both  together. 

6.  Minor  Third :  eg  on  the  lower,  or  on  both  together ;  hd/  on  both  together. 

7.  Whole  Tone  [major  Tone]  :  cd  and  c'd'  on  both  together  [the  minor  Tone 
is  produced  by  d  and  e  on  both  together]. 

8.  Semitone  [diatonic  Semitone]  :  6  c'  on  the  upper. 

When  both  tones  are  produced  from  the  same  disc  the  objective  combinational 
tones  are  very  powerful,  as  has  been  already  remarked,  p.  157a.  But  if  the  tones 
are  produced  from  different  discs,  the  combinational  tones  are  weak.  In  the  latter 
case,  (and  this  is  the  chief  point  of  interest  to  us  at  present)  the  two  tones  can 
be  made  to  act  together  with  any  desired  difference  of  phase.  This  is  effected  by 
altering  the  position  of  the  upper  box.  ^ 

We  have  first  to  investigate  the  phenomena  as  they  occur  in  the  unison  gg. 
The  effect  of  the  interference  of  the  two  tones  in  this  case  is  complicated  by  the 
fact  that  the  siren  produces  cq^pound  and  not  simple  tones  and  that  the  in- 
terference of  individual  partial  tones  is  independent  of  that  of  the  prime  tone 
and  of  one  another.  In  order  to  damp  the  upper  partial  tones  in  the  siren  by 
means  of  a  resonance  chamber,  I  caused  cylindrical  boxes  of  brass  to  be  made, 
of  which  the  back  hcjves  are  shewn  at  hj  h,  and  ho  ho  fig.  56,  opposite.  These 
boxes  are  each  made  in  two  sections,  so  that  they  can  be  removed,  and  be  again 
attached  to  the  windchest  by  means  of  screws.  When  the  tone  of  the  siren 
approaches  the  prime  tone  of  these  boxes,  its  quality  becomes  full,  strong  and  soft, 
like  a  fine  tone  on  the  French  horn ;  otherwise  the  siren  has  rather  a  piercing  tone. 
At  the  same  time  we  use  a  small  quantity  of  air,  but  considerable  pressure.  The 
circumstances  are  of  the  same  nature  as  when  a  tongue  is  applied  to  a  resonance 
chamber  of  the  same  pitch.  Used  in  this  way  the  siren  is  very  well  adapted  for  ^ 
experiments  on  interference. 

If  the  boxes  are  so  placed  that  the  puffs  of  air  follow  at  exactly  equal  intervals 
from  both  discs,  similar  phases  of  the  prime  tone  and  of  all  partials  coincide,  and 
aU  are  reinforced. 

If  the  handle  is  turned  round  half  a  right  angle,  the  upper  box  is  turned  round 
I  of  a  right  angle,  or  ^  of  the  circumference,  that  is  half  the  distance  between 
the  holes  in  the  series  of  12  holes  which  is  in  action  for  g.  Hence  the  difference 
in  the  phase  of  the  two  primes  is  half  the  vibrational  period,  the  puffs  of  air  in 
one  box  occur  exactly  in  the  middle  between  those  of  the  other,  and  the  two 
prime  tones  mutually  destroy  each  other.  But  under  these  circumstances  the 
difference  of  phase  in  the  upper  Octave  is  precisely  the  whole  of  the  vibrational 
period ;  that  is,  they  reinforce  each  other,  and  similarly  all  the  evenly  numbered 
harmonic  upper  partials  reinforce  each  other  in  the  same  position,  and  the  unevenly 
numbered  ones  destroy  each  other.  Hence  in  the  new  position  the  tone  is  weaker,  ^ 
because  deprived  of  several  of  its  partials  ;  but  it  does  not  entirely  cease ;  it  rather 
jumps  up  an  Octave.  If  we  further  turn  the  handle  through  another  half  a  right 
angle  so  that  the  box  is  turned  through  a  whole  right  angle,  the  puffs  of  the  two 
discs  again  agree  completely,  and  the  tones  reinforce  one  another.  Hence  in  a 
complete  revolution  of  the  handle  there  are  four  positions  where  the  whole  tone  of 
the  siren  appears  reinforced,  and  four  intermediate  positions  where  the  prime  tone 
and  all  uneven  upper  partials  vanish,  and  consequently  the  Octave  occurs  in  a 
weaker  form  accompanied  by  the  evenly  numbered  upper  partials.  If  we  attend  to 
the  first  upper  partial,  which  is  the  Octave  of  the  prime,  by  listening  to  it  through 
a  proper  resonator,  we  find  that  it  vanishes  after  turning  through  a  quarter  of  a 
right  angle,  and  is  reinforced  after  turning  through  half  a  right  angle,  and  hence 
for  every  complete  revolution  of  the  handle  it  vanishes  8  times,  and  is  reinforced 
8  times.  The  third  partial,  (or  second  wpper  partial,)  the  Twelfth  of  the  prime 
tone,  vanishes  in  the  same  time  12  times,  the  fourth  partial  16  times,  and  so  on. 

Digitized  by  VjO€)^l€' 


1 64  ORIGIN  OP  BEATS.  pabt  ii. 

Other  compound  tones  behave  likelihose  of  the  siren.  When  two  tones  of  the 
same  pitch  are  sounded  together  having  differences  of  phase  corresponding  to  half 
the  periodic  time,  the  tone  does  not  vanish,  but  jumps  up  an  Octave.  When,  for 
example,  two  open  organ  pipes,  or  two  reed  pipes  of  the  same  construction  and 
pitch,  are  placed  beside  each  other  on  the  same  portvent,  their  vibrations  generally 
accommodate  themselves  in  such  a  manner  that  the  stream  of  air  enters  first  one 
and  then  the  other  alternately ;  and  while  the  tone  of  stopped  pipes,  which  have 
only  unevenly  numbered  partials,  is  then  almost  entirely  destroyed,  the  tone  of  the 
open  pipes  and  reed  pipes  falls  into  the  upper  Octave.  This  is  the  reason  why  no 
reinforcement  of  tone  can  be  effected  on  an  organ  or  harmonium  by  combining 
tongues  or  pipes  of  the  same  kind,  [on  the  same  portvent]. 

So  far  we  have  combined  tones  of  precisely  the  same  pitch;  now  let  us  inquire 
what  happens  when  the  tones  have  slightly  different  pitch.     The  double  siren 

%  just  described  is  also  well  fitted  for  explaining  this  case,  for  we  can  slightly  alter 
the  pitch  of  the  upper  tone  by  slowly  revolving  the  upper  box  by  means  of  the 
handle,  the  tone  becoming  flatter  when  the  direction  of  revolution  is  the  same  as 
that  of  the  disc,  and  sharper  when  it  is  opposite  to  the  same.  The  vibrational 
period  of  a  tone  of  the  siren  is  equal  to  the  time  required  for  a  hole  in  the  rotating 
disc  to  pass  from  one  hole  in  the  windbox  to  the  next.  If,  through  the  rotation  of 
the  box,  the  hole  of  the  box  advances  to  meet  the  hole  of  the  disc,  the  two  holes 
come  into  coincidence  sooner  than  if  the  box  were  at  rest :  and  hence  the  vibra- 
tional period  is  shorter,  and  the  tone  sharper.  The  converse  takes  place  when  the 
revolution  is  in  the  opposite  direction.  These  alterations  of  pitch  are  easily  heard 
when  the  box  is  revolved  rather  quickly.  Now  produce  the  tones  of  twelve  holes 
on  both  discs.  These  will  be  in  absolute  unison  as  long  as  the  upper  box  is  at 
rest.  The  two  tones  constantly  reinforce  or  enfeeble  each  other  according  to  the 
position  of  the  upper  box.    But  on  setting  the  upper  box  in  motion,  the  pitch  of 

%  the  upper  tone  is  altered,  while  that  of  the  lower  tone,  which  has  an  inmiovable 
windbox,  is  unchanged.  Hence  we  have  now  two  tones  of  slightly  different  pitch 
sounding  together.  And  we  hear  the  so-called  beats  of  the  tones,  that  is,  the 
intensity  of  the  tone  will  be  alternately  greater  and  less  in  regular  succession.*  The 
arrangement  of  our  siren  makes  the  reason  of  this  readily  intelligible.  The 
revolution  of  the  upper  box  brings  it  alternately  in  positions  which  as  we  have 
seen  correspond  to  stronger  and  weaker  tones.  When  the  handle  has  been  turned 
through  a  right  angle,  the  windbox  passes  from  a  position  of  loudness  through  a 
position  of  weakness  to  a  position  of  strength  again.  Consequently  every  complete 
revolution  of  the  handle  gives  us  four  beats,  whatever  be  the  rate  of  revolution  of 
the  discs,  and  hence  however  low  or  high  the  tone  may  be.  If  we  stop  the  box  at 
the  moment  of  maximum  loudness,  we  continue  to  hear  the  loud  tone ;  if  at  a 
moment  of  minimum  force,  we  continue  to  hear  the  weak  tone. 

The  mechanism  of  the  instrument  also  explains  the  connection  between  the 

f  number  of  beats  and  the  difference  of  the  pitch.  It  is  easily  seen  that  the  number 
of  the  puffs  is  increased  by  one  for  every  quarter  revolution  of  the  handle.    But 

V  every  such  quarter  revolution  corresponds  to  one  beat.  Hence  the  number  of  beats 
in  a  given  time  is  equal  to  the  difference  of  the  numbers  of  vibrations  executed  by 
the  two  tones  in  the  same  time.  This  is  the  general  law  which  determines  the 
number  of  beats,  for  all  kinds  of  tones.  This  law  results  immediately  from  the 
construction  of  the  siren ;  in  other  instruments  it  can  only  be  verified  by  very 
accurate  and  laborious  measurements  of  the  numbers  of  vibrations. 

The  process  is  shewn  graphically  in  fig.  57.  Here  c  c  represents  the  series  of 
puffs  belonging  to  one  tone,  and  d  d  those  belonging  to  the  other. '  The  distance 
for  c  c  is  divided  into  18  parts,  the  same  distance  is  divided  into  20  parts  for  d  d.  At 

*  [The  German  word  Schioebuna,  which  '  beat.'  But  it  is  not  asnal  to  make  the  dis- 
might  be  rendered  '  fluotuation/  implies  this :  tinotion  in  English,  where  the  whole  pheno- 
The  loudest  portion  only  is  called  the  StosSy  or      menoni     ailed  hea,iB,^Translator,] 


Digitized  by  VjOOQIC 


CHAP.  VIII.  ORIGIN  OF  BEATS.  165 

^»  3»  5.  both  puffs  concur,  and  the  tone  is  reinforced.  At  2  and  4  they  are  inter- 
mediate and  mutually  enfeeble  each  other.  The  number  of  beats  for  the  whole 
distance  is  2,  because  the  difference  of  the  numbers  of  parts,  each  of  which  cor- 
respond to  a  vibration,  is  also  2. 

The  intensity  of  tone  varies ;  swelling  from  a  minimum  to  a  maximum,  and 
lessening  from  the  maximum  to  the  TniniTnnTn.    It  is  the  places  of  maximum 


FIO.  57. 


.^/«     ....  ^.       ...'?...,.  4,       ...      ^^ 


intensity  which  are  properly  called  beats,  and  these  are  separated  by  more  or  less 
distinct  pauses. 

Beats  are  easily  produced  on  all  musical  instruments,  by  striking  two  notes  of  ^ 
nearly  the  same  pitch.  They  are  heard  best  from  the  simple  tones  of  tuning-forks 
or  stopped  organ  pipes,  because  here  the  tone  really  vanishes  in  the  pauses.  A 
little  fluctuation  in  the  pitch  of  the  beating  tone  may  then  be  remarked.*  For  the 
compound  tones  of  other  instruments  the  upper  partial  tones  are  heard  in  the 
pauses,  and  hence  the  tone  jumps  up  an  Octave,  as  in  the  case  of  interference 
already  described.  If  we  have  two  tuning-forks  of  exactly  the  same  pitch,  it  is 
only  necessary  to  stick  a  little  wax  on  to  the  end  of  one,  to  strike  both,  and  bring 
them  near  the  same  ear  or  to  the  surface  of  a  table,  or  sounding  board.  To  make 
two  stopped  pipes  beat,  it  is  only  necessary  to  bring  a  flnger  slowly  near  to  the  lip 
of  one,  and  thus  flatten  it.  The  beats  of  compound  tones  are  heard  by  striking 
any  note  on  a  pianoforte  out  of  tune  when  the  two  strings  belonging  to  the  same 
note  are  no  longer  in  unison ;  or  if  the  piano  is  in  tune  it  is  sufficient  to  attach  a 
piece  of  wax,  about  the  size  of  a  pea,  to  one  of  the  strings.  This  puts  them  suffi- 
ciently out  of  tune.  More  attention,  however,  is  required  for  compound  tones  ^ 
because  the  enfeeblement  of  the  tone  is  not  so  striking.  The  beat  in  this  case 
resembles  a  fluctuation  in  pitch  and  quality.  This  is  very  striking  on  the  siren 
according  as  the  brass  resonance  cylinders  (ho  ho  and  h^  h^  of  fig.  56,  p.  162)  are 
attached  or  not.  These  make  the  prime  tone  relatively  strong.  Hence  when  beats 
are  produced  by  turning  a  handle,  the  decrease  and  increase  of  loudness  in  the  tone 
is  very  striking.  On  removing  the  resonance  cylinders,  the  upper  partial  tones 
are  relatively  powerful,  and  since  the  ear  is  very  uncertain  when  comparing  the 
loudness  of  tones  of  different  pitch,  the  alteration  of  force  during  the  beats  is 
much  less  striking  than  that  of  pitch  and  quality  of  tone. 

On  listening  to  the  upper  partials  of  compound  tones  which  beat,  it  will  be 
found  that  these  beat  also,  and  that  for  each  beat  of  the  prime  tone  there  are  two 
of  the  second  partial,  three  of  the  third,  and  so  on.  Hence  when  the  upper  partials 
are  strong,  it  is  easy  to  make  a  mistake  in  counting  the  beats,  especially  when  the 
beats  of  the  primes  are  very  slow,  so  that  they  occur  at  intervals  of  a  second  or  two.  IT 
It  is  then  necessary  to  pay  great  attention  to  the  pitch  of  the  beats  counted,  and 
sometimes  to  apply  a  resonator. 

It  is  possible  to  render  beats  visible  by  setting  a  suitable  elastic  body  into 
sympathetic  vibration  with  them.  Beats  can  then  occur  only  when  the  two 
exciting  tones  lie  near  enough  to  the  prime  tone  of  the  sympathetic  body  for  the 
latter  to  be  set  into  sensible  sympathetic  vibration  by  both  the  tones  used.  This 
is  most  easily  done  with  a  thin  string  which  is  stretched  on  a  sounding  board 
on  which  have  been  placed  two  tuning-forks,  both  of  very  nearly  the  same  pitch 
as  the  string.  On  observing  the  vibrations  of  the  string  through  a  microscope, 
or  attaching  a  fibril  of  a  goosefeather  to  the  string  which  will  make  the  same 
excursions  on  a  magnified  scale,  the  string  will  be  clearly  seen  to  make  sympathetic 

^  See  the  explanation  of  this  phenomenon      French  translator  of   this  work,]  in  Appen- 
which  was  given  me  by  Mons.  G.  Gu6roult,  [the      dix  XIV. 


Digitized  by 


Google 


i66  ORIGIN  OP  BEATS.  part  n. 

vibrations  with  alternately  large  and  small  excursions,  according  as  the  tone  of  the 
two  forks  is  at  its  maximum  or  minimum. 

The  same  effect  can  be  obtained  from  the  sympathetic  vibration  of  a  stretched 
membrane.    Fig.  58  is  the  copy  of  a  drawing  made  by  a  vibrating  membrane  of 

PfG.  58. 


this  sort,  used  in  the  phonautograph  of  Messrs.  Scott  &  Eoenig,  of  Paris.  The  mem- 
brane of  this  instrument,  which  resembles  the  drumskin  of  the  ear,  carries  a  small 
stiff  style,  which  draws  the  vibrations  of  the  membrane  upon  a  rotating  cylinder. 
In  the  present  case  the  membrane  was  set  in  motion  by  two  organ  pipes,  that  beat. 

^  The  undulating  line,  of  which  only  a  part  is  here  given,  shews  that  times  of  strong 
vibration  have  alternated  with  times  of  almost  entire  rest.  In  this  case,  then,  the 
beats  are  also  sympathetically  executed  by  the  membrane.  Similar  drawings 
again  have  been  made  by  Dr.  Politzer,  who  attached  the  writing  style  to  the 
auditory  bone  (the  columella)  of  a  duck,  and  then  produced  a  beating  tone  by 
means  of  two  organ  pipes.  This  experiment  shewed  that  even  the  auditory  bones 
follow  the  beats  of  two  tones.* 

Generally  this  must  always  be  the  case  when  the  pitches  of  the  two  tones 
struck  differ  so  little  from  each  other  and  from  that  of  the  proper  tone  of  the  sym- 
pathetic body,  that  the  latter  can  be  put  into  sensible  vibration  by  both  tones  at 
once.  Sympathetic  bodies  which  do  not  damp  readily,  such  as  tuning-forks, 
consequently  require  two  exciting  tones  which  differ  extraordinarily  little  in  pitch, 
in  order  to  shew  visible  beats,  and  the  beats  must  therefore  be  very  slow.  For 
bodies  readily  damped,  as  membranes,  strings,  &c.,  the  difference  of  the  exciting 

f  tones  may  be  greater,  and  consequently  the  beats  may  succeed  each  other  more 
rapidly. 

This  holds  also  for  the  elastic  terminal  formations  of  the  auditory  nerve  fibres. 
Just  as  we  have  seen  that  there  may  be  visible  beats  of  the  auditory  ossicles,  Gorti's 
arches  may  also  be  made  to  beat  by  two  tones  sufficiently  near  in  pitch  to  set  the 
same  Gorti's  arches  in  sympathetic  vibration  at  the  same  time.  If  then,  as  we 
have  previously  supposed,  the  intensity  of  auditory  sensation  in  the  nerve  fibres 
involved  increases  and  decreases  with  the  intensity  of  the  elastic  vibrations,  the 
strength  of  the  sensation  must  also  increase  and  diminish  in  the  same  degree  as  the 
vibrations  of  the  corresponding  elastic  appendages  of  the  nerves.  In  this  case  also 
the  motion  of  Gorti's  arches  must  still  be  considered  as  compounded  of  the  motions 
which  the  two  tones  would  have  produced  if  they  had  acted  separately.  According 
as  these  motions  are  directed  in  the  same  or  in  opposite  directions  they  will  rein- 
force or  enfeeble  each  other  by  (algebraical)  addition.    It  is  not  till  these  motions 

f  excite  sensation  in  the  nerves  that  any  deviation  occurs  from  the  law  that  each  of 
the  two  tones  and  each  of  the  two  sensations  of  tones  subsist  side  by  side  without 
disturbance. 

We  now  come  to  a  part  of  the  investigation  which  is  very  important  for  the 

[  theory  of  musical  consonance,  and  has  also  unfortunately  been  little  regarded  by 

;  acousticians.  The  question  is  :  what  becomes  of  the  beats  when  they  grow  Caster 
and  faster  ?  and  to  what  extent  may  their  number  increase  without  the  ear  being 
unable  to  perceive  them  ?  Most  acousticians  were  probably  inclined  to  agree  with 
the  hypothesis  of  Thomas  Young,  that  when  the  beats  became  very  quick  they 
gradually  passed  over  into  a  combinational  tone  (the  first  differential).  Young 
imagined  that  the  pulses  of  tone  which  ensue  during  beats,  might  have  the  same 

*  The  beats  of  two  tones  are  also  clearly  tones.    Even  withont  using  the  rotating  mirror 

shewn  by  the  vibrating  flame  described  at  the  for  observing  the  flames,  we  can  easily  reoog- 

end  of  Appendix  II.    The  flame  must  be  con-  nise  the  alterations  in  the  shape  of  the  flaihe 

nected  with  a  resonator  having  a  pitch  suffi-  which  take  place  isoohronously  with  the  aiidible 

ciently  near  to  those  of  the  two  generating  beats. 

Digitized  by  V^OOQIC 


oHAP.viii.        LIMITS  OP  THE  FREQUENCY  OP  BEATS.  167 

effect  on  the  ear  as  elementary  pnlses  of  air  (in  the  siren,  for  example),  and  that 
just  as  30  puffs  in  a  second  tlu*ough  a  siren  would  produce  the  sensation  of  a  deep 
tone,  so  would  30  beats  in  a  second  resulting  from  any  two  higher  tones  produce 
the  same  sensation  of  a  deep  tone.  Certainly  this  view  is  well  supported  by  the 
fact  that  the  vibrational  number  of  the  first  and  strongest  combinational  tone  is 
actually  the  number  of  beats  produced  by  the  two  tones  in  a  second.  It  is,  however, 
of  much  importance  to  remember  that  there  are  other  combinational  tones  (my 
summational  tones),  which  will  not  agree  with  this  hypothesis  in  any  respect,* 
but  on  the  other  hand  are  readily  deduced  from  the  theory  of  combinational  tones 
which  I  have  proposed  (in  Appendix  XII.).  It  is  moreover  an  objection  to  Young's 
theory,  that  in  many  cases  the  combinational  tones  exist  extem^y  to  the  ear,  and  ' 
are  able  to  set  properly  tuned  membranes  or  resonators  into  sympathetic  vibra- 
tion,t  because  this  could  not  possibly  be  the  case,  if  the  combinational  tones  were 
nothing  but  the  series  of  beats  with  undisturbed  superposition  of  the  two  waves.  ^ 
For  the  mechanical  theory  of  sympathetic  vibration  shews  that  a  motion  of  the 
air  compounded  of  two  simple  vibrations  of  different  periodic  times,  is  capable  of 
putting  such  bodies  only  into  sympathetic  vibration  as  have  a  proper  tone  corre- 
sponding to  one  of  the  two  given  tones,  provided  no  conditions  intervene  by  which 
the  simple  superposition  of  two  wave  systems  might  be  disturbed ;  and  the  nature 
of  such  a  disturbance  was  investigated  in  the  last  chapter.^  Hence  we  may ; 
consider  combinational  tones  as  an  accessory  phenomenon,  by  which,  however,  the 
course  of  the  two  primary  wave  systems  and  of  their  beats  is  not  essentially 
interrupted. 

Against  the  old  opinion  we  may  also  adduce  the  testimony  of  our  senses,  which 
teaches  us  that  a  much  greater  number  of  beats  than  30  in  a  second  can  be 
distinctly  heard.  To  obtain  this  result  we  must  pass  gradually  from  the  slower  to 
the  more  rapid  beats,  taking  care  that  the  tones  chosen  for  beating  are  not  too  far 
apart  from  each  other  in  the  scale,  because  audible  beats  are  not  produced  unless  ^ 
the  tones  are  so  near  to  each  other  in  the  scale  that  they  can  both  make  the  same 
elastic  appendages  of  the  nerves  vibrate  sympathetically .§  The  number  of  beats, 
however,  can  be  increased  without  increasing  the  interval  between  the  tones,  if 
both  tones  are  taken  in  the  higher  octaves. 

The  observations  are  best  begun  by  producing  two  simple  tones  of  the  same 
pitch,  say  from  the  once-accented  octave  by  means  of  tuning-forks  or  stopped  organ 
pipes,  and  slowly  altering  the  pitch  of  one.  This  is  effected  by  sticking  more  and 
more  wax  on  one  of  the  forks ;  or  more  and  more  covering  the  mouth  of  one  of 
the  pipes.  Stopped  organ  pipes  are  also  generally  provided  with  a  movable  plug 
or  lid  at  the  stopped  end,  in  order  to  tune  them ;  by  pulling  this  out  we  flatten,  by 
pushing  it  in  we  sharpen  the  tone.** 

When  a  slight  difference  in  pitch  has  been  thus  produced,  the  beats  are  heard 
at  first  as  long  drawn  out  fluctuations  alternately  swelling  and  vanishing.    Slow 
beats  of  this  kind  are  by  no  means  disagreeable  to  the  ear.    In  executing  music  ^ 
containing  long  sustained  chords,  they  may  even  produce  a  solemn  effect,  or  else 
give  a  more  lively,  tremulous  or  agitating  expression.    Hence  we  find  in  modem 

♦   [Prof.  Preyer  shews,  App.  XX.  sect.  L.  tion  of  the  following  facts,  is  made  with  two 

art.  4,  c2,  that  summational  tones,  as  snggested  *  pitch  pipes,*  each  consisting  of  an  extensible 

by  Appmm,  may  be  considered  as  differential  stopped  pipe,  which  has  the  compass  of  the 

tones  of  the  second  order,  ii  such  are  admitted,  once-accented  octave  and  is  blown  as  a  whistle, 

— Translaior.^  the  two  being  connected  by  a  bent  tabe  with 

t   [After  the  experiments  of  Prof.  Preyer  a  single  mouthpiece.    By  carefully  adjusting 

and  Mr.  Bosanqnet,  App.  XX.  sect.  L.  art.  4,  the  lengths  of  the  pipes,  I  was  first  able  to  pro- 

this  mast  be  considered  as  due  to  some  error  dace  complete  destruction  of  the  tone  by  inter- 

of  observation. — Translator.]  ference,  the  sound  returning  immediately  when 

X  [See  Bosanquet*s  theory  of  *  transforma-  the  mouth  of  one  whistle  was  stopped  by  the 

tion  '  in  App.  XX.  sect.  L.  art.  5,  a.— Trans-  finger.     Then  on  gradually  lengthening  one  of 

Jator.]  the  pipes  the  beats  began  to  be  heard  slowly, 

§  [Eoenig  knows  no  such  limitation.    See  and  increased  in  rapidity.     The  tone  being 

App.  XX.  sect.  L.  art.  3. — Translator,]  nearly  simple   the    beats   are  well  heard. — 

**  [A  cheap  apparatus,  useful  for  demonstra-  Translator.] 

Digitized  by  V^OOQ IC 


i68  LIMITS  OP  THE  FREQUENCY  OF  BEATS.  paot  ii. 

organs  and  harmoniums,  a  stop  with  two  pipes  or  tongues,  adjusted  to  heat.  This 
imitates  the  trembling  of  the  human  voice  and  of  violins  which,  appropriately  in- 
troduced in  isolated  passages,  may  certainly  be  very  expressive  and  effective,  but 
apphed  continuously,  as  is  unfortunately  too  common,  is  a  detestable  malpractice. 

f  The  ear  easily  follows  slow  beats  of  not  more  than  4  to  6  in  a  second.  The 
hearer  has  time  to  apprehend  all  their  separate  phases,  and  become  conscious  of 
each  separately,  he  can  even  count  them  without  difficulty.*  But  when  the  interval 
between  the  two  tones  increases  to  about  a  Semitone,  the  number  of  beats  becomes 
20  or  30  in  a  second,  and  the  ear  is  consequently  unable  to  follow  them  sufficiently 
well  for  counting.  If,  however,  we  begin  with  hearing  slow  beats,  and  then  increase 
their  rapidity  more  and  more,  we  cannot  fiEul  to  recognise  that  the  sensational  im- 
pression on  the  ear  preserves  precisely  the  same  character,  appearing  as  a  series 
of  separate  pulses  of  sound,  even  when  their  frequency  is  so  great  that  we  have 

^  no  longer  time  to  fix  each  beat,  as  it  passes,  distinctly  in  our  consciousness  and 
count  it.t 

But  while  the  hearer  in  this  case  is  quite  capable  of  distinguishing  that  his  ear 
now  hears  30  beats  of  the  same  kind  as  the  4  or  6  in  a  second  which  he  heard 
before,  the  effect  of  the  collective  impression  of  such  a  rapid  beat  is  quite  different. 
In  the  first  place  the  mass  of  tone  becomes  confused,  which  I  principally  refer  to 
the  psychological  impressions.  We  actuaUy  hear  a  series  of  pulses  of  tone,  and 
are  able  to  recognise  it  as  such,  although  no  longer  capable  of  following  each 
singly  or  separating  one  from  the  other.  But  besides  this  psychological  considera- 
tion, the  sensible  impression  is  also  unpleasant.  Such  rapidly  beating  tones  are 
jarring  and  rough.  The  distinctive  property  of  jarring,  is  the  intermittent  cha- 
racter of  the  sound.  We  think  of  the  letter  B  as  a  characteristic  example  of 
a  jarring  tone.  It  is  well  known  to  be  produced  by  interposing  the  uvula,  or  else 
the  thin  tip  of  the  tongue,  in  the  way  of  the  stream  of  air  passing  out  of  the  mouth, 

^  in  such  a  manner  as  only  to  allow  the  air  to  force  its  way  through  in  separate  pulses, 
the  consequence  being  that  the  voice  at  one  time  sounds  freely,  and  at  another  is 
cut  off.  j: 

Intermittent  tones  were  also  produced  on  the  double  siren  just  described  by 
using  a  Uttle  reed  pipe  instead  of  the  vdnd-conduit  of  the  upper  box,  and  driving 
the  air  through  this  reed  pipe.  The  tone  of  this  pipe  can  be  heard  externally  only 
when  the  revolution  of  the  disc  brings  its  holes  before  the  holes  of  the  box  and 
open  an  exit  for  the  air.  Hence,  if  we  let  the  disc  revolve  while  air  is  driven 
through  the  pipe,  we  obtain  an  intermittent  tone,  which  sounds  exactly  like  beats 
arising  firom  two  tones  sounded  at  once,  although  the  intermittence  is  produced  by 
purely  mechanical  means.  Such  effects  may  also  be  produced  in  another  way  on 
the  same  siren,  Eemove  the  lower  windbox  and  retain  only  its  pierced  cover, 
over  which  the  disc  revolves.  At  the  under  part  apply  one  extremity  of  an  india- 
rubber  tube  against  one  of  the  holes  in  the  cover,  the  other  end  being  conducted 

f  by  a  proper  ear-piece  to  the  observer's  ear.  The  revolving  disc  alternately  opens 
and  closes  the  hole  to  which  the  india-rubber  tube  has  been  applied.  Hold  a 
tuning-fork  in  action  or  some  other  suitable  musical  instrument  above  and  near 

*  [See  App.  XX.  sect.  B.  No.  7,  for  direc-  Octave,  but  become   rapidly  too  fast  to  be 

tions  for  observing  heata.— Translator.]  follo^eed.    As,  however,  these  are  not  simple 

t  [The  Harmonical  is  very  convenient  for  tones,  the   beats  are   not  perfectly  olear. — 

this  purpose.    On  the  db  key  is  a  d,  one  Translator  J] 

comma  lower  than  d.  These  dd^  beat  about  %  [Phonautographio  figures  of  the  effect 
9,  18,  36,  73  times  in  10  seconds  in  the  of  r,  resemble  those  of  fig.  58,  p.  i66a.  8ix 
different  Octaves,  the  last  barely  countable,  varieties  of  these  figures  are  given  on  p.  19  of 
Also  e'b  and  e,  beat  33,  66,  132,  364  in  10  .Donders's  important  little  tract,  on 'The  Physio- 
seconds  in  the  different  Octaves.  The  two  fiirst  llogy  of  Speech  Sounds,  and  especially  of  those 
of  these  sets  of  beats  can  be  counted,  the  two  /  in  the  Dutch  Language  *  {De  Physioloaie  der 
last  cannot  be  counted,  but  will  be  distinctly  /  Spraakklanken^  in  het  bijeonder  van  ate  der 
perceived  as  separate  pulses.    Similarly  the  \  nederlandsche  taal,    Utrecht  1870,  pp.  24). — 


beats  between  all  consecutive  notes,  (except  F   '  Translator,] 
and  Of  B  and  C),  can  be  counted  in  the  lowest 


-^.^v 


Digitized  by 


Google 


CHAP.  vin.       LIMITS  OP  THE  FREQUENCY  OP  BEATS.  [t6gj 

the  rotating  disc.  Its  tone  will  be  heard  intermittently  and  the  number  of 
intermissions  can  be  regulated  by  altering  the  velocity  of  the  rotation  of  the 
disc. 

In  both  ways  then  we  obtain  intermittent  tones.  In  the  first  case  the  tone  of 
the  reed  pipe  as  heard  in  the  outer  air  is  interrupted,  because  it  can  only  escape 
from  time  to  time.  The  intermittent  tone  in  this  case  can  be  heard  by  any  number 
of  listeners  at  once.  In  the  second  case  the  tone  in  the  outer  air  is  continuous, 
but  reaches  the  ear  of  the  observer,  who  hears  it  through  the  disc  of  the  siren, 
intermittently.  It  can  certainly  be  heard  by  one  observer  only,  but  then  aU  kinds 
of  musical  tones  of  the  most  diverse  pitch  and  quality  may  be  employed  for  the 
purpose.  The  intermission  of  their  tones  gives  them  all  exactly  the  same  kind  of 
roughness  which  is  produced  by  two  tones  which  beat  rapidly  together.  We  thus 
come  to  recognise  clearly  that  beats  and  intermissions  are  identical,  and  that  either 
when  fast  enough  produces  what  is  termed  a  jar  or  rattle.  f 

Beats  produce  intermittent  excitement  of  certain  auditory  nerve  fibres.    The 
refikson  why  such  an  intermittent  excitement  acts  so  much  more  unpleasantly  than 
an  equally  strong  or  even  a  stronger  continuous  excitement,  may  be  gathered  from  ^j^  ^ 
the  analogous  action  of  other  human  nerves.    Any  powerful  excitement  of  a  nerve  ^*" 
deadens  its  excitability,  and  consequently  renders  it  less  sensitive  to  fresh  irritants,  ^j^  ] 
But  after  the  excitement  ceases,  and  the  nerve  is  left  to  itself,  irritabihty  is  speedily  ^'i 
re-established  in  a  living  body  by  the  influence  of  arterial  blood.    Fatigue  and  re-       X^^^ 
freshment  apparently  supervene  in  different  organs  of  the  body  with  different 
velocities ;  but  they  are  found  wherever  muscles  and  nerves  have  to  operate.    The 
eye,  which  has  in  many  respects  the  greatest  analogy  to  the  ear,  is  one  of  those 
organs  in  which  both  fatigue  and  refreshment  rapidly  ensue.    We  need  to  look  at 
the  sun  but  an  instant  to  find  that  the  portion  of  the  retina,  or  nervous  expansion 
of  the  eye,  which  was  affected  by  the  solar  light  has  become  less  sensitive  for  other 
light.    Immediately  afterwards  on  turning  our  eyes  to  a  uniformly  illuminated  ^ 
surface,  as  the  sky,  we  see  a  dark  spot  of  the  apparent  size  of  the  sun ;  or  several 
such  spots  with  lines  between  them,  if  we  had  not  kept  our  eye  steady  when  look- 
ing at  the  sun  but  had  moved  it  right  and  lefk.    An  instant  suffices  to  produce  this 
effect ;  nay,  an  electric  spark,  that  lasts  an  immeasurably  short  time,  is  fully 
capable  of  causing  this  species  of  fatigue. 

When  we  continue  to  look  at  a  bright  surface,  the  impression  is  strongest  at 
first,  but  at  the  same  time  it  blunts  the  sensibility  of  the  eye,  and  consequently 
the  impression  becomes  weaker,  the  longer  we  allow  the  eye  to  act.  On  coming 
out  of  darkness  into  full  daylight  we  feel  blinded ;  but  after  a  few  minutes,  when 
the  sensibility  of  the  eye  has  been  blunted  by  the  irritation  of  the  light, — or,  as  we 
say,  when  the  eye  has  grown  accustomed  to  the  glare, — this  degree  of  brightness  is 
found  very  pleasant.  Conversely,  in  coming  from  fall  daylight  into  a  dark  vault, 
we  are  insensible  to  the  weak  light  about  us,  and  can  scarcely  find  our  way  about, 
yet  after  a  few  minutes,  when  the  eye  has  rested  from  the  effect  of  the  strong  hght,  ^ 
we  are  able  to  see  very  well  in  the  semi-dark  room. 

These  phenomena  and  the  like  can  be  conveniently  studied  in  the  eye,  because 
individual  spots  in  the  eye  may  be  excited  and  others  left  at  rest,  and  the  sensations 
of  each  may  be  afterwards  compared.  Put  a  piece  of  black  paper  on  a  tolerably 
well-lighted  white  surface,  look  steadily  at  a  point  on  or  near  the  black  paper,  and 
then  withdraw  the  paper  suddenly.  The  eye  sees  a  secondary  image  of  the  black 
paper  on  the  white  surfebce,  consisting  of  that  portion  of  the  white  surfetce  where 
the  black  paper  lay,  which  now  appears  brighter  than  the  rest.  The  place  in  the 
eye  where  the  image  of  the  black  paper  had  been  formed,  has  been  rested  in  com- 
parison with  all  those  places  which  had  been  affected  by  the  white  surface,  and 
on  removing  the  black  paper  this  rested  part  of  the  eye  sees  the  white  surface  in 
its  first  fresh  brightness,  while  those  parts  of  the  retina  which  had  been  already 
fatigued  by  looking  at  it,  see  a  decidedly  greyer  tinge  on  the  whiter  surfeuse. 

Hence  by  the  continuous  uniform  action  of  the  irritation  of  light,  this  irritation 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQ IC 


■/ 


I70  LIMITS  OP  THE  PEEQUENOY  OP  BEATS*  pabt  n. 

itself  blunts  the  sensibility  of  the  nerve,  and  thus  effeotually  protects  this  organ 
against  too  long  and  too  violent  excitement. 

It  is  quite  different  when  we  allow  intermittent  light  to  act  on  the  eye,  such  as 
flashes  of  light  with  intermediate  pauses.  During  these  pauses  the  sensibility  is 
again  somewhat  re-established,  and  the  new  irritation  consequently  acts  much 
more  intensely  than  if  it  had  lasted  with  the  same  uniform  strength.  Every  one 
knows  how  unpleasant  and  annoying  is  any  flickering  light,  even  if  it  is  relatively 
very  weak,  coming,  for  example,  from  a  little  flickering  taper  or  rushlight. 

The  same  thing  holds  for  the  nerves  of  touch.  Scraping  with  the  nail  is  far 
more  annoying  to  the  skin  than  constant  pressure  on  the  same  place  with  the 
same  pressure  of  the  nail.  The  unpleasantness  of  scratching,  rubbing,  tickling 
depends  upon  the  intermittent  excitement  which  they  produce  in  the  nerves  of 
touch, 
f  A  jarring  intermittent  tone  is  for  the  nerves  of  hearing  what  a  flickering  light 
is  to  the  nerves  of  sight,  and  scratching  to  the  nerves  of  touch.  A  much  more 
intense  and  unpleasant  excitement  of  the  organs  is  thus  produced  than  would  be 
occasioned  by  a  continuous  uniform  tone.  This  is  even  shewn  when  we  hear  very 
weak  intermittent  tones.  If  a  tuning-fork  is  struck  and  held  at  such  a  distance 
from  the  ear  that  its  sound  cannot  be  heard,  it  becomes  immediately  audible  if  the 
handle  of  the  fork  be  revolved  by  the  Angers.  The  revolution  brings  it  alternately 
into  positions  where  it  can  and  cannot  transmit  sound  to  the  ear  [p.  161&],  and 
this  alternation  of  strength  is  immediately  perceptible  by  the  ear.  For  the  same 
reason  one  of  the  most  deUcate  means  of  hearing  a  very  weak,  simple  tone  consists 
in  sounding  another  of  nearly  the  same  strength,  which  makes  from  2  to  4  beats  in 
a  second  with  the  first.  In  this  case  the  strength  of  the  tone  varies  from  nothing 
to  4  times  the  strength  of  the  single  simple  tone,  and  this  increase  of  strength 
combines  with  the  alternation  to  make  it  audible, 
f  Just  as  this  alternation  of  strength  will  serve  to  strengthen  ihe  impression  of 
the  very  weakest  musical  tones  upon  the  ear,  we  must  conclude  that  it  must  also 
serve  to  make  the  impression  of  stronger  tones  much  more  penetrating  and  violent, 
than  they  would  be  if  their  loudness  were  continuous. 

We  have  hitherto  confined  our  attention  to  cases  where  the  number  of  beats 
did  not  exceed  20  or  30  in  a  second.  We  saw  that  the  beats  in  the  middle  part  of 
the  scale  are  still  quite  audible  and  form  a  series  of  separate  pulses  of  tone.  But 
this  does  not  furnish  a  Umit  to  their  number  in  a  second. 

The  interval  V  c"  gave  us  33  beats  in  a  second,  and  the  effect  of  sounding  the  two 

notes  together  was  very  jarring.    The  interval  of  a  whole  tone  b^}  c"  gives  nearly 

twice  as  many  beats,  but  these  are  no  longer  so  cutting  as  the  former.    The  rule 

assigns  88  beats  in  a  second  to  the  minor  Third  a'  c'^  but  in  reality  this  interval 

scarcely  shews  any  of  the  roughness  produced  by  beats  from  tones  at  closer  intervals. 

We  might  then  be  led  to  conjecture  that  the  increasing  number  of  beats  weakened 

^  their  impression  and  made  them  inaudible.    This  conjecture  would  find  an  analogy 

in  the  impossibiUty  of  separating  a  series  of  rapidly  succeeding  impressions  of 

light  on  the  eye,  when  their  number  in  a  second  is  too  large.    Think  of  a  Rowing 

stick  swung  round  in  a  circle.    If  it  executes  10  or  15  revolutions  in  a  second,  the 

eye  believes  it  sees  a  continuous  circle  of  fire.    Similarly  for  colour-tops,  with 

which  most  of  my  readers  are  probably  familiar.    If  the  top  be  spun  at  the  rate 

of  more  than  10  revolutions  in  a  second,  the  colours  upon  it  mix  and  form  a  per- 

/  fectly  unchanging  impression  of  a  mixed  colour.    It  is  only  for  very  intense  light 

'  that  the  alternations  of  the  various  fields  of  colour  have  to  take  place  more  quickly, 

I    20  to  30  times  in  a  second.    Hence  the  phenomena  are  quite  analogous  for  ear  and 

{    eye.    When  the  alternation  between  irritation  and  rest  is  too  feist,  the  alternation 

ceases  to  be  felt,  and  sensation  becomes  continuous  and  lasting. 

However,  we  may  convince  ourselves  that  in  the  case  of  the  ear,  an  increase  of 
the  number  of  beats  in  a  second  is  not  the  only  cause  of  the  disappearance  of  the 


Digitized  by  VjOOQIC 


CHAP.  VIII.        LIMITS  OP  THE  FREQUENCY  OF  BEATS.  171 

corresponding  sensation.    As  we  passed  from  the  interval  of  a  Semitcme  V  c"  to 
that  of  a  minor  Third  a'  c",  we  not  only  increased  the  number  of  beats,  but  the 
width  of  the  interval.    Now  we  can  increase  the  nmnber  of  beats  without  increasing 
the  interval  by  taking  it  in  a  higher  Octave.    Thus  taking  V  d'  an  Octave  higher 
we  have  V  c'"  with  66  beats,  and  another  Octave  would  give  us  V"  c""  with  as 
many  as  132  beats,  and  these  are  really  audible  in  the  same  way  as  the  33  beats 
of  V  cf'y  although  they  certainly  become  weaker  in  the  higher  positions.    Never- 
theless the  66  beats  of  the  interval  h"  d"  are  much  more  distinct  and  penetrating 
than  the  same  number  in  the  whole  Tone  h'\}c"t  and  the  ^Z  of  the  interval  e'"  f" 
are  still  quite  evident,  while  the  88  of  the  minor  Third  a'  c"  are  practically  in- 
audible.   My  assertion  that  as  many  as  132  beats  in  a  second  are  audible  will  per- 
haps appear  very  strange  and  incredible  to  acousticians.    But  the  experiment  is 
easy  to  repeat,  and  if  on  an  instrument  which  gives  sustained  tones,  as  an  organ 
or  harmonium,  we  strike  a  series  of  intervals  of  a  Semitone  each,  beginning  low  % 
down,  and  proceeding  higher  and  higher,  we  shall  hear  in  the  lower  parts  very 
slow  beats  {Bfi  gives  4I,  B  c  gives  8^,  b  d  gives  16^  beat  in  a  second),  and  as  we 
ascend  the  rapidity  will  increase  but  the  character  of  the  sensation  remain  un- 
altered.   And  thus  we  can  pass  gradually  from  4  to  132  beats  in  a  second,  and 
convince  ourselves  that  though  we  become  incapable  of  counting  them,  their  cha- 
racter as  a  series  of  pulses  of  tone,  producing  an  intermittent  sensation,  remains 
unaltered.    It  must  be  observed,  however,  that  the  beats,  even  in  the  higher  parts  ' ' 
of  the  scale,  become  much  shriller  and  more  distinct,  when  their  number  is    \ 
diminished  by  taking  intervals  of  quarter  tones  or  less.    The  most  penetrating     i 
roughness  arises  even  in  the  upper  parts  of  the  scale  from  beats  of  30  to  40  in  a 
second.    Hence  high  tones  in  a  chord  are  much  more  sensitive  to  an  error  in 
tuning  amounting  to  the  fraction  of  a  Semitone,  than  deep  ones.    While  two  d 
notes  which  differ  from  one  another  by  the  tenth  part  of  a  Semitone,  produce  about 
3  beats  in  two  seconds^*  which  cannot  be  observed  without  considerable  attention,  f 
and,  at  least,  gives  notfeeling  of  roughness,  two  d'  notes  with  the  same  error  give 
3  beats  in  one  second,  and  two  c"'  notes  6  beats  in  one  second,  which  becomes  very 
disagreeable.    The  character  of  the  roughness  also  alters  with  the  number  of  beats.  1 
Slow  beats  give  a  coarse  kind  of  roughness,  which  may  be  considered  as  chattering  1 
or  jarring ;  and  quicker  ones  have  a  finer  but  more  cutting  roughness. 

Hence  it  is  not,  or  at  least  not  solely,  the  large  number  of  beats  which  renders 
them  inaudible.  The  magnitude  of  the  interval  is  a  £a.ctor  in  the  result,  and  con- 
sequently we  are  able  with  high  tones  to  produce  more  rapid  audible  beats  than 
with  low  tones. 

Observation  shews  us,  then,  on  the  one  hand,  that  equally  large  intervals  by 
no  means  give  equally  distinct  beats  in  all  parts  of  the  scale.  The  increasing 
number  of  beats  in  a  second  renders  the  beats  in  the  upper  part  of  the  scale  less 
distinct.  The  beats  of  a  Semitone  remain  distinct  to  the  upper  limits  of  the  four- 
times  accented  octave  [say  4000  vib.],  and  this  is  also  about  the  limit  for  musical  ^ 
tones  fit  for  the  combinations  of  hannony.  The  beats  of  a  whole  tone,  which  in 
deep  positions  are  very  distinct  and  powerfal,  are  scarcely  audible  at  the  upper 
limit  of  the  thrice-accented  octave  [say  at  2000  vib.].  The  major  and  minor 
Third,  on  the  other  hand,  which  in  the  middle  of  the  scale  [264  to  528  vib.]  may 
be  regarded  as  consonances,  and  when  justly  intoned  scarcely  shew  any  roughness, 
are  decidedly  rough  in  the  lower  octaves  and  produce  distinct  beats. 

On  the  other  hand  we  have  seen  that  distinctness  of  beating  and  the  roughness 
of  the  combined  sounds  do  not  depend  solely  on  the  number  of  beats.  For  if  we 
could  disregard  their  magnitudes  all  the  following  intervals,  which  by  calculation 
should  have  33  beats,  would  be  equally  rough : 

*  [Taking  c'»264,  a  tone  one-tenth  of  a      second.    The  figures  in  the  text  have  bee 
Semitone  or  10  cents  higher  miJce  265-5  vibra-      altered  to  these  more  exact  numbers.— 2VaiM- 
tions,  and  these  tones  beat  i}  times  in  a      2a^.] 

Digitized  by  VjOOQIC 


172 


LIMITS  OP  THE  FREQUENCY  OP  BEATS. 


PABT  n. 


t^ 


[528-495=33] 

[major,  297-264]  and  S  e'  [minor  330-297] 

[198-165] 

[165-132] 

[132-99] 


the  tones  with  ^  of  its  own 

between  the  intensities  of  vibration  o  and  -^. 


the  Semitone         .        •    h'  c" 

the  whole  Tones    .        .    d  W 

the  minor  Third    .         .    e  gf 

the  major  Third     .         .    ce 

the  Fourth     .        .        .    Gc 

the  Fifth       .        .        .    CQ  [99-66] 
and  yet  we  find  that  these  intervals  are  more  and  more  free  from  roughness.* 

The  roughness  arising  from  sounding  two  tones  together  depends,  then,  in  a 
compound  manner  on  the  magnitude  of  the  interval  and  the  number  of  beats  pro- 
duced in  a  second.  On  seeking  for  the  reason  of  this  dependence,  we  observe  that, 
as  before  remarked,  beats  in  the  ear  can  exist  only  when  two  tones  are  produced 
sufficiently  near  in  the  scale  to  set  the  same  elastic  appendages  of  the  auditory 
nerve  in  sympathetic  vibration  at  the  same  time.  When  the  two  tones  produced 
^  are  too  far  apart,  the  vibrations  excited  by  both  of  them  at  once  in  Corti*s  organs 
are  too  weak  to  admit  of  their  beats  being  sensibly  felt,  supposing  of  course  that 
no  upper  partial  or  combinational  tones  intervene.  According  to  the  assumptions 
made  in  the  last  chapter  respecting  the  degree  of  damping  possessed  by  Corti's 
organs  (p.  144c),  it  would  result,  for  example,  that  for  the  interval  of  a  whole  Tone 
c  dy  such  of  Corti's  fibres  as  have  the  proper  tone  c|!,  would  be  excited  by  each  of 

intensity;  and  these  fibres  will  therefore  fluctuate 
But  if  we  strike  the  simple  tones  c 
and  d$fy  it  follows  from  the  table  there  given  that  Corti's  fibres  which  correspond 
to  the  middle  between  c  and  oj)!  will  alternate  between  the  intensities  o  and  |f. 
Conversely  the  same  intensity  of  beats  would  for  a  minor  Third  amount  to  only 
0*194,  and  for  a  major  Third  to  only  0*108,  and  hence  would  be  scarcely  perceptible 
beside  the  two  primary  tones  of  the  intensity  i. 
Pig«  59»  which  we  used  on  p.  144^^  to  express  the 
%  intensity  of  the  sympathetic  vibration  of  Corti's 
fibres  for  an  increasing  interval  of  tone,  may 
here  serve  to  shew  the  intensity  of  the  beats 
which  two  tones  excite  in  the  ear  when  forming 
different  intervals  in  the  scale.  But  the  parts  on 
the  base  line  must  now  be  considered  to  repre- 
sent fifths  of  a  wlioU  Tone,  and  not  as  before  of 
a  Semitone.    In  the  present  case  the  distance  of 

the  two  tones  from  each  other  is  doubly  as  great  as  that  between  either  of  them 
and  the  intermediate  Corti*s  fibres. 

Had  the  damping  of  Corti's  organs  been  equally  great  at  all  parts  of  the  scale, 
and  had  the  number  of  beats  no  influence  on  the  roughness  of  the  sensation,  equal 
intervals  in  all  parts  of  the  scale  would  have  given  equal  roughness  to  the  combined 
tones.  But  as  this  is  not  the  case,  as  the  same  intervals  diminish  in  roughness 
%  as  we  ascend  in  the  scale,  and  increase  in  roughness  as  we  descend,  we  must  either 
assume  that  the  damping  power  of  Corti's  organs  of  higher  pitch  is  less  than  that 
of  those  of  lower  pitch,  or  else  that  the  discrimination  of  the  more  rapid  beats 
meets  with  certain  hindrances  in  the  nature  of  the  sensation  itself. 

At  present  I  see  no  way  of  deciding  between  these  two  suppositions ;  but  the 
former  is  possibly  the  more  improbable,  because,  at  least  with  our  artificial  musical 
instruments,  the  higher  the  pitch  of  a  vibrating  body,  the  more  difficulty  is  ex- 
perienced in  isolating  it  sufficiently  to  prevent  it  from  communicating  its  vibrations 
to  its  environment.  Very  short,  high-pitched  strings,  little  metal  tongues  or  plates, 
&c.,  yield  high  tones  which  die  off  with  great  rapidity,  whereas  it  is  easy  to 
generate  deep  tones  with  correspondingly  greater  bodies  which  shall  retain  their 
tone  for  a  considerable  time.  On  the  other  hand  the  second  supposition  is  favoured 
by  the  analogy  of  another  nervous  apparatus,  the  eye.    As  has  been  already  re- 

the  student  shonld  listen  to  the  beats  of  the 
primes  only.— TrawZator.] 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


♦  [All  these  intervals  can  be  tried  on  the 
Harmonical,  but  as  the  tones  are  compound, 


CHAP.  vin.        LIMITS  OF  THE  FREQUENCY  OF  BEATS. 


173 


■f 


marked,  a  series  of  impressions  of  light,  following  each  other  rapidly  and  regularly, 
excite  a  uniform  and  continuous  sensation  of  light  in  the  eye.  When  the  separate 
luminous  irritations  follow  one  another  very  quickly,  the  impression  produced  by 
each  one  lasts  unweakened  in  the  nerves  till  the  next  supervenes,  and  thus  the 
pauses  can  no  longer  be  distinguished  in  sensation.  In  the  eye,  the  number  of 
separate  irritations  cannot  exceed  24  in  a  second  without  being  completely  fused 
into  a  single  sensation.  In  this  respect  the  eye  is  far  surpassed  by  the  ear,  which 
can  distinguish  as  many  as  132  intermissions  in  a  second,  and  probably  even  that 
is  not  the  extreme  limit.  Much  higher  tones  of  sufficient  strength  would  probably 
allow  us  to  hear  still  more.*  It  lies  in  the  nature  of  the  thing,  that  different  kinds 
of  apparatus  of  sensation  should  shew  different  degrees  of  mobility  in  this  respect, 
since  the  result  does  not  depend  simply  on  the  mobility  of  the  molecules  of  the 
nerves,  but  also  depends  upon  the  mobility  of  the  auxiliary  apparatus  through 
which  the  excitement  is  induced  or  expressed.  Muscles  are  much  less  mobile  than  ^ 
the  eye ;  ten  electrical  discharges  in  a  second  directed  through  them  generally 
suffice  to  bring  the  voluntary  muscles  into  a  permanent  state  of  contraction.  For 
the  muscles  of  the  involuntary  system,  of  the  bowels,  the  vessels,  &c.,  the  pauses 
between  the  irritations  may  be  as  much  as  one,  or  even  several  seconds  long,  with- 
out any  intermission  in  the  continuity  of  contraction. 

The  ear  is  greatly  superior  in  this  respect  to  any  other  nervous  apparatus.  It 
is  eminently  the  organ  for  small  intervals  of  time,  and  has  been  long  used  as  such 
by  astronomers.  It  is  well  known  that  when  two  pendulums  are  ticking  near  one 
another,  the  ear  can  distinguish  whether  the  ticks  are  or  are  not  coincident,  within 
one  hundredth  of  a  second.  The  eye  would  certainly  fail  to  determine  whether 
two  flashes  of  light  coincided  within  ^^  second ;  and  probably  within  a  much  larger 
fraction  of  a  second.f 

But  although  the  ear  shews  its  superiority  over  other  organs  of  the  body  in 
this  respect,  we  cannot  hesitate  to  assume  that,  like  every  other  nervous  apparatus,  ^ 
the  rapidity  of  its  power  of  apprehension  is  limited,  and  we  may  even  assume  that 
we  have  approached  very  near  the  limit  when  we  can  but  faintly  distinguish  132 
beats  in  a  second. 


*  [In  the  two  high  notes  g""  f"'t  of  the 
flageolet  fifes  (p.  153(2,  note),  which  if  justly 
intoned  should  give  198  beats  in  a  second,  I 
could  hear  none,  though  the  tones  were  very 
powerful,  and  the  scream  was  very  cutting 
indeed* — ^In  the  case  of  V  d'\  which  on  the 
Harmonical  are  tuned  to  make  1056  and  990, 
the  rattle  of  the  66  beats,  or  thereabouts,  is 
quite  distinct,  and  the  differential  tone  is  very 
powerful  at  the  same  time. — Translator.] 

f  [The  following  is  an  interesting  compari- 
son between  eye  and  ear,  and  eye  and  hand. 
The  usual  method  of  observing  transits  is  by 
counting  the  pendulum  ticks  of  an  astronomi- 
cal clock,  and  by  observing  the  distances  of 
the  apparent  positions  of  a  star  before  and  after 
passing  each  bar  of  the  transit  instrument  at 
the  moments  of  ticking,  to  estimate  the  moment 
at  which  it  had  passed  each  bar.  This  is  done 
for  five  bars  and  a  mean  is  taken.  But  a  few 
years  ago  a  chronograph  was  introduced  at 
Greenwich  Observatory,  consisting  of  a  uni- 
formly revolving  cylinder  in  which  a  point 
pricks  a  hole  every  second.  Electrical  com- 
munication being  established  with  a  knob  on 
the  transit  instrument,  the  observer  presses 
the  knob  at  the  moment  he  sees  a  star  dis- 


appear behind  a  bar,  and  an  electrical  current 
causes  another  point  to  make  a  hole  between 
the  seconds  holes  on  the  chronograph.  By 
applying  a  scale,  the  time  of  transit  is  thus 
measured  off.  A  mean,  of  course,  is  taken  as 
before.  On  my  asking  Mr.  Stone  (now  Astrono- 
mer at  Oxford,  then  chief  assistant  at  Green- 
wich Observatory)  as  to  the  relative  degree  of 
accuracy  of  the  two  methods,  he  told  me  that 
he  considered  the  first  gave  results  to  one- 
tenth,  and  the  second  to  one-twentietii  of  a 
second.  It  must  be  remembered  that  the  first 
method  also  required  a  mental  estimation 
which  had  to  be  performed  in  less  than  a  m 
second,  and  the  result  borne  in  mind,  and  that 
this  was  avoided  by  the  second  plan.  On  the 
other  hand  in  the  latter  the  sensation  had  to 
be  conveyed  from  the  eye  to  the  brain,  which 
issued  its  orders  to  the  hand,  and  the  hand 
had  to  obey  them.  Hence  there  was  an  endea- 
vour at  performing  simultaneously,  several 
acts  which  could  only  be  successive.  Any  one 
will  find  upon  trial  that  an  attempt  to  merely 
make  a  mark  at  the  moment  of  hearing  an 
expected  sound,  as,  for  example,  the  repeated 
tick  of  a  common  half  seconds  clock,  is  liable 
to  great  error. — Translator,] 


Digitized  by 


Google 


174  DEEP  AND  DEEPEST  TONES.  part  n. 


CHAPTEE  IX. 

DEEP  AND  DEEPEST  TONES. 

Beats  give  us  an  important  means  of  determining  the  limit  of  the  deepest  tones, 
and  of  accounting  for  certain  peculiarities  of  the  transition  from  the  sensation  of 
separate  pulses  of  air  to  a  perfectly  continuous  musical  tone,  and  to  this  inquiry 
we  now  proceed. 

The  question :  what  is  the  smallest  number  of  vibrations  in  a  second  which 
can  produce  the  sensation  of  a  musical  tone  ?  has  hitherto  received  very  contra- 
dictory replies.    The  estimates  of  different  observers  fluctuate  between  8  (Savort) 

^  and  about  30.  The  contradiction  is  explained  by  the  existence  of  certain  difScul- 
ties  in  the  experiments. 

In  the  first  place  it  is  necessary  that  the  strength  of  the  vibrations  of  the  air 
for  very  deep  tones  should  be  extremely  greater  than  for  high  tones,  if  they  are  to 
make  as  strong  an  impression  on  the  ear.  Several  acousticians  have  occasionally 
started  the  hypothesis  that,  caeteris  pariJms,  the  strength  of  tones  of  different 
heights  is  directly  proportional  to  the  vis  viva  of  the  motion  of  the  air,  or,  which 
comes  to  the  same  thing,  to  the  amount  of  the  mechanical  work  applied  for  pro- 
ducing it.  But  a  simple  experiment  with  the  siren  shews  that  when  equal  amounts 
of  mechanical  work  are  applied  to  produce  deep  and  high  tones  under  conditions 
otherwise  alike,  the  high  tones  excite  a  very  much  more  powerful  sensation  than 
the  deep  ones.  Thus,  if  the  siren  is  blown  by  a  bellows,  which  makes  its  disc 
revolve  with  increasing  rapidity,  and  if  we  take  care  to  keep  up  a  perfectly 
uniform  motion  of  the  bellows  by  raising  its  handle  by  the  same  amount  the  same 

^  number  of  times  in  a  minute,  so  as  to  keep  its  bag  equally  filled,  and  drive  the 
same  amount  of  air  under  the  same  pressure  through  Ifee  siren  in  the  same  time, 
we  hear  at  first,  while  the  revolution  is  slow,  a  weaJc  deep  tone,  which  continually 
ascends,  but  at  the  same  time  gains  strength  at  an  extraordinary  rate,  till  when  the 
highest  tones  producible  on  my  double  siren  (near  to  a",  with  880  vibrations  in  a 
second)  are  reached,  their  strength  is  almost  insupportable.  In  this  case  by  £ar 
the  greatest  part  of  the  uniform  mechanical  work  is  applied  to  the  generation  of 
sonorous  motion,  and  only  a  small  part  can  be  lost  by  the  friction  of  the  revolving 
disc  on  its  axial  supports,  and  the  air  which  it  sets  into  a  vortical  motion  at  the 
same  time ;  and  these  losses  must  even  be  greater  for  the  more  rapid  rotation  than 
for  the  slower,  so  that  for  the  production  of  the  high  tones  less  mechanical  work 
remains  applicable  than  for  the  deep  ones,  and  yet  the  higher  tones  appear  to  our 
sensation  extraordinarily  more  powerful  than  the  deep  ones.  How  far  upwards 
this  increase  may  extend,  I  have  as  yet  been  unable  to  determine,  for  the  rapidity 

^  of  my  siren  cannot  be  farther  increased  with  the  same  pressure  of  air. 

The  increase  of  strength  with  height  of  tone  is  of  especial  consequence  in  the 
deepest  part  of  the  scale.  It  follows  that  in  compound  tones  of  great  depth,  the 
upper  partial  tones  may  be  superior  to  the  prime  in  strength,  even  though  in 
musical  tones  of  the  same  description,  but  of  greater  height,  the  strength  of  the 
prime  greatly  predominates.  This  is  readily  proved  on  my  double  siren,  because 
by  means  of  the  beats  it  is  easy  to  determine  whether  any  partial  tone  which  we 
hear  is  the  prime,  or  the  second  or  third  partial  tone  of  the  compound  under 
examination.  For  when  the  series  of  12  holes  are  open  in  both  windboxes,  and 
the  handle,  which  moves  the  upper  windbox,  is  rotated  once,  we  shall  have,  as 
already  shown,  4  beats  for  the  primes,  8  for  the  second  partials,  and  12  for  the 
third  partials.  Now  we  can  make  the  disc  revolve  more  slowly  than  usual,  by 
allowing  a  well-oiled  steel  spring  to  rub  against  the  edge  of  one  isc  with  different 
degrees  of  pressure,  and  thus  we  can  easily  produce  series  of  puffs  which  corre- 
spond to  very  deep  tones,  and  then,  turning  the  handle,  we  can  count  the  beats. 


Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


CHAP.  IX.  DEEP  AND  DEEPEST  TONES.  175 

By  allowing  the  rapidity  of  the  revolution  of  the  discs  to  increase  gradually,  we 
find  that  the  first  audible  tones  produced  make  1 2  beats  for  each  revolution  of  the 
handle,  the  number  of  puffs  being  firom  36  to  40  in  the  second.  For  tones  with 
from  40  to  80  puffs,  each  revolution  of  the  handle  gives  8  beats.  In  this  case, 
then,  the  upper  Octave  of  the  prime  is  the  strongest  tone.  It  is  not  till  we  have 
80  puffs  in  a  second  that  we  hear  the  four  beats  of  the  primes. 

It  is  proved  by  these  experiments  that  motions  of  the  air,  which  do  not  take 
the  form  of  pendular  vibrations,  can  excite  distinct  and  powerful  sensations  of  tone, 
of  which  the  pitch  number  is  2  or  3  times  the  number  of  the  pulses  of  the  air, 
and  yet  that  the  prime  tone  is  not  heard  through  them.  Hence,  when  we  continu- 
ally descend  in  the  scale,  the  strength  of  our  sensation  decreases  so  rapidly  that 
the  sound  of  the  prime  tone,  although  its  vis  viva  is  independently  greater  than  that 
of  the  upper  partials,  as  is  shewn  in  higher  positions  of  a  musical  tone  of  the 
same  composition,  is  overcome  and  concealed  by  its  own  upper  partials.  Even  ^ 
when  the  action  of  the  compound  tone  on  the  ear  is  much  reinforced,  the  effect 
remains  the  same.  In  the. experiments  with  the  siren  the  uppermost  plate  of  the 
bellows  is  violently  agitated  for  the  deep  tones,  and  when  I  laid  my  head  on  it,  my 
whole  head  was  set  into  such  powerful  sympathetic  vibration  that  the  holes  of  the 
rotating  disc,  which  vanish  to  an  eye  at  rest,  became  again  separately  visible, 
through  an  optical  action  similar  to  that  which  takes  place  in  stroboscopic  discs. 
The  row  of  holes  in  action  appeared  to  stand  still,  the  other  rows  seemed  to  move 
partly  backwards  and  partly  forwards,  and  yet  the  deepest  tones  were  no  more 
distinct  than  before.  At  another  time  I  connected  my  ear  by  means  of  a  properly 
introduced  tube  with  an  opening  leading  to  the  interior  of  the  bellows.  The 
agitation  of  the  drum  skin  of  the  ear  was  so  great  that  it  produced  an  intolerable 
itching,  and  yet  the  deepest  tones  remained  as  indistinct  as  ever. 

In  order,  then,  to  discover  the  limit  of  deepest  tones,  it  is  necessary  not  only  to 
produce  very  violent  agitations  in  the  air  but  to  give  these  the  form  of  simple  % 
pendular  vibrations.  Until  this  last  condition  is  fulfilled  we  cannot  possibly  say 
whether  the  deep  tones  we  hear  belong  to  the  prime  tone  or  to  an  upper  partial  tone 
of  the  motion  of  the  air.*  Among  the  instruments  hitherto  employed  the  wide- 
stopped  organ  pipes  are  the  most  suitable  for  this  purpose.  Their  upper  partial 
tones  are  at  least  extremely  weak,  if  not  quite  absent.  Here  we  find  that  even  the 
lower  tones  of  the  16-foot  octave,  C,  to  E^^  begin  to  pass  over  into  a  droning  noise, 
so  that  it  becomes  difficult  for  even  a  practised  musical  ear  to  assign  their  pitch  with 
certainty ;  and,  indeed,  they  cannot  be  tuned  by  the  ear  alone,  but  only  indirectly 
by  means  of  the  beats  which  they  make  with  the  tones  of  the  upper  octaves.  We 
observe  a  similar  effect  on  the  same  deep  tones  of  a  piano  or  harmonium ;  they 
form  drones  and  seem  out  of  tune,  although  their  musical  character  is  on  the 
whole  better  established  than  in  the  pipes,  because  of  their  accompanying  upper 
partial  tones.  In  music,  as  artistically  applied  in  an  orchestra,  the  deepest  tone 
used  is,  therefore,  the  E,  of  the  [4-stringed  German]  double  bass,  with  41^  vibra-  f 
tions  in  a  second,  [see  p.  i8c,  note]  and  I  think  I  may  predict  with  certainty  that  all 
efforts  of  modem  art  applied  to  produce  good  musical  tones  of  a  lower  pitch  must 
fail,  not  because  proper  means  of  agitating  the  air  cannot  be  discovered,  but 
because  the  human  ear  cannot  hear  them.  The  16-foot  C,  of  the  organ,  with 
33  vibrations  in  a  second,  certainly  gives  a  tolerably  continuous  sensation  of 
drone,  but  does  not  allow  us  to  give  it  a  definite  position  in  the  musical  scale. 
We  almost  begin  to  observe  the  separate  pulses  of  air,  notwithstanding  the  regular 
form  of  the  motion.  In  the  upper  half  of  the  32 -foot  octave,  the  perception  of  the 
separate  pulses  becomes  still  clearer,  and  the  continuous  part  of  the  sensation, 

*  Thos  Savart's  instrument,  where  a  rota-  tion,  and  consequently  the  upper  partial  tones 

ting  rod  strikes  through  a  narrow  slit,  is  totally  must    be    very  strongly  developed,  and  the 

unsuitable  for  making  the  lowest  tone  audible,  deepest  tones,  which  are  heard  for  8  to  16 

The  separate  puSs  of  air  are  here  very  short  in  passages  of  the  rod  through  the  hole  in  a  second, 

relation  to  the  whole  periodic  time  of  the  vibra-  can  be  nothing  but  upper  partials. 

Digitized  by  VjOOQlC 


176 


DEEP  AND  DEEPEST  TONES* 


PART  IK 


which  may  be  compared  with  a  sensation  of  tone,  continaally  weaker,  and  in  Uiq 
lower  half  of  the  3  2 -foot  octave  we  can  scarcely  be  said  to  hear  anything  but  the 
individual  pulses,  or  if  anything  else  is  really  heard,  it  can  only  be  weak  upper 
partial  tones,  from  which  the  musical  tones  of  stopped  pipes  are  not  quite  free. 

I  have  tried  to  produce  deep  simple  tones  in  another  way.  Strings  which  are 
weighted  in  their  middle  with  a  heavy  piece  of  metal,  on  being  struck  give  a  com- 
pound tone  consisting  of  many  simple  tones  which  are  mutually  inharmonic.  The 
prime  tone  is  separated  from  the  nearest  upper  partials  by  an  interval  of  several 
Octaves,  and  hence  there  is  no  danger  of  confusing  it  with  any  of  them  ;  besides, 
the  upper  tones  die  away  rapidly,  but  the  deeper  ones  continue  for  a  very  long  time. 
A  string  of  this  kind*  was  stretched  on  a  sounding-box  having  a  single  opening 
which  could  be  connected  with  the  auditory  passage,  so  that  the  air  of  the  sounding- 
box  could  escape  nowhere  else  but  into  the  ear.  The  tones  of  a  string  of  customary 
m  pitch  are  under  these  circumstances  insupportably  loud.  But  for  D„  with  37^ 
vibrations  in  a  second,  there  was  only  a  very  weak  sensation  of  tone,  and  even  this 
was  rather  jarring,  leading  to  the  conclusion  that  the  ear  began  even  here  to  feel 
the  separate  pulses  separately,  notwithstanding  their  regularity.  At  5^^,  with 
29^  vibrations  in  a  second,  there  was  scarcely  anything  audible  left.  It  appears, 
then,  that  those  nerve  fibres  which  perceive  such  tones  begin  as  early  as  at  this 
note  to  be  no  longer  excited  with  a  uniform  degree  of  strength  during  the  whole 
time  of  a  vibration,  whether  it  be  the  phases  of  greatest  velocity  or  the  phases  of 
greatest  deviation  from  their  mean  position  in  the  vibrating  formations  in  the  ear 
which  effect  the  excitement.f 


*  It  was  a  thin  brass  pianoforte  string.  The 
weight  was  a  copper  kreutzer  piece,  [pronounce 
kroitser;  three  kreutzers  make  a  penny  at 
Heidelberg,  where  the  experiment  was  pro- 
bably tried,]  pierced  in  the  middle  by  a  hole 
«r  through  which  the  wire  passed,  and  then  made 
''  to  grip  the  wire  immovably  by  driving  a  steel 
point  between  the  hole  in  the  kreutzer  and  the 
string. 

f  Subsequently  I  obtained  two  large  tuning- 
forks  from  Herr  Koenig  in  Paris,  with  sliding 
weights  on  their  prongs.  By  altering  the  posi- 
tion of  the  weights,  the  pitch  was  changed, 
and  the  corresponding  number  of  vibrations 
was  given  on  a  scale  which  runs  along  the 
prongs.  One  fork  gave  24  to  35,  the  other  35 
to  61  vibrations.  The  sliding  weight  is  a  plate, 
5  centimetres  [nearly  2  inches]  in  diameter, 
and  forms  a  mirror.  On  bringing  the  ear  close 
to  these  plates  the  deep  tones  are  well  heard. 
For  30  vibrations  I  could  still  hear  a  weak 
drone,  for  28  scarcely  a  trace,  although  this 
arrangement  made  it  easily  possible  to  form 
«r  oscillations  of  9  millimetres  [about  ^  inch]  in 
amplitude,  quite  close  to  the  ear.  Prof.  W. 
Preyer  has  been  thus  able  to  hear  down  to  24 
vib.  He  has  also  applied  another  method 
(Pkysioloffische  Ahhandlungen^  Physiological 
Treatises,  Series  i,  part  i,  *0n  the  limits  of 
the  perception  of  tone,'  pp.  1-17)  by  using  very 
deep,  loaded  tongues,  in  reed  pipes,  which  were 
constructed  for  this  purpose  by  Herr  Appunn 
of  Hanau,  and  gave  from  8  to  40  vib.  These 
were  set  into  strong  vibration  by  blowing,  and 
then  on  interrupting  the  wind,  the  dying  off 
of  the  vibrations  was  listened  to  by  laying  the 
ear  against  the  box.  He  states  that  tones  were 
heard  downwards  as  low  as  15  vib.  But  the 
proof  that  the  tones  heard  corresponded  with 
the  primes  of  the  pipes  depends  only  on  the 
fact,  that  the  pitch  gradually  ascended  as  they 
passed  over  into  the  tones  of  from  25  to  32 
vib.:  which  were  more  audible,  but  died  off  more 


rapidly.  With  extensive  vibrations,  however, 
the  tongues  may  have  very  easily  given  their 
point  of  attachment  longitudinal  impulses  of 
double  the  frequency,  because  when  they 
reached  each  extremity  of  their  amplitude  they 
might  drive  back  the  point  of  attachment 
through  their  flexion,  whereas  in  the  middle 
of  the  vibration  they  would  draw  it  forward  by 
the  centrifugal  force  of  their  weight.  Since 
the  power  of  distinguishing  pitch  for  these 
deepest  tones  is  extremely  imperfect,  I  do  not 
feel  my  doubts  removed  by  the  judgment  of 
the  ear  when  the  estimates  are  not  checked  by 
the  counting  of  beats. 

[This  check  I  am  fortunately  able  to  supply. 
A  copy  of  the  instrument  used  by  Prof.  Preyer 
is  in  the  South  Kensington  Museum.  It  con- 
sists of  an  oblong  box,  in  the  lower  part  of 
which  are  the  loaded  harmonium  reeds,  not 
attached  to  pipes,  but  vibrating  within  the  box, 
and  governed  by  valves  which  can  be  opened 
at  pleasure.  On  account  of  the  beats  between 
tongue  and  tongue  taking  place  in  strongly 
condensed  air,  they  are  accelerated,  and  the 
nominal  pitch,  obtained  by  counting  the  beats 
from  reed  to  reed,  is  not  quite  the  same 
as  the  actual  pitch  (see  App.  XX.  sect.  B. 
No.  6).  The  series  of  tones  is  supposed  to 
proceed  from  8  to  32  vib.  by  differences  of  i 
vib.,  from  32  to  64  by  differences  of  2  vib.,  and 
from  64  to  128  by  differences  of  4  vibs.  In 
November  1879,  for  another  purpose,  I  deter- 
mined the  pitch  of  every  reed  by  Scheibler's 
forks,  (see  App.  XX.  sect.  B.  No.  7)  by  means 
of  the  upper  partials  of  the  reeds.  For  Beeds 
8,  9,  xo,  II,  I  used  from  the  20th  to  the  30U1 
partial,  but  I  consider  only  Beed  11  as  quite 
certain.  I  found  it  made  10-97  ^i^-  ^y  ^he  20th, 
and  10-95  by  both  the  21st  and  24th  partiids. 
From  Heed  11  upwards  I  determined  every 
pitch,  in  many  cases  by  several  partials,  the 
result  only  differing  in  the  second  place  of 
decimals.    I  give  the  two  lowest  Octaves,  the 


Digitized  by  VjOOQlC 


CHAP.  IX. 


DEEP  AND  DEEPEST  TONES. 


^77 


Hence  although  tones  of  24  to  28  vib.  have  been  heard,  notes  do  not  begin  to 
/have  a  definite  pitch  till  about  40  vibrations  are  performed  in  a  second.  These 
facts  will  agree  with  the  hypothesis  concerning  the  elastic  appendages  to  the  audi- 
tory nerves,  on  remembering  that  tlie  deeply  intoned  fibres  of  Corti  may  be  set  in 
sympathetic  vibration  by  still  deeper  tones,  although  with  rapidly  decreasing 
strength,  so  that  sensation  of  tone,  but  no  discrimination  of  pitch,  is  possible.  If 
the  most  deeply  intoned  of  Corti's  fibres  lie  at  greater  intervals  from  each  other  in 
the  scale,  but  at  the  same  time  their  damping  power  is  so  great  that  every  tone 
which  corresponds  to  the  pitch  of  a  fibre,  also  pretty  strongly  affects  the  neighbour- 
ing fibres,  there  will  be  no  safe  distinction  of  pitch  in  their  vicinity,  but  it  will 
proceed  continuously  without  jumps,  while  the  intensity  of  the  sensation  must  at 
the  same  time  become  small. 

Whilst  simple  tones  ia  the  upper  half  of  the  16-foot  octave  are  perfectly  con- 


only  pitches  of  interest  for  the  present  pur- 
pose, premising  that  I  consider  the  three  lowest 
pitches  (for  which  the  upper  partials  lay  too 


close  together)  and  the  highest  (which  had  a  H 
bad  reed)  to  be  very  uncertain. 


Nominal 
Actual 

Nominal 
Actual 

Nominal 
Actual 


8        9  10  II  12  13  14  15  16 

791    889  981  10-95  11-90  12-90  13-93  14-91  15-91 

17      18  19  20  21  22  23  24  25 

16*90 17-91  18-89  J9'9i  20-91  21-91  22-88  23-97  24-92 

26      27  28  29  30  31  32 

25-9226-86  2785  28-84  29-77  30-68  31-47 


There  can  therefore  be  no  question  as  to  the 
real  pitch.  At  Prof.  Preyer's  request  I  ex- 
amined this  instrument  in  Oct.  1877,  >^Qd  he 
has  printed  my  notes  in  his  Akustische  Unter- 
suchungen,  pp.  6-8.  From  these  I  extract  the 
following : — 

R  means  Reed,  and  R  21  "25  means  that  the 
two  reeda  21  and  25  were  sounded  together  and 
gave  beats. 

R  21-25,  beat  4  in  i  sec,  counted  for  20  sec. 
Henoe  both  of  their  lowest  partials  must  have  been 
effective. 

R  20-24,  beat  4  in  i  sec,  counted  for  10  sec. 

R  19 ••23,  beat  4  in  i  sec,  counted  for  20  sec. 

R  17  ••21,  same  beats. 

R  16  ••20,  same  beats  quite  distinctly. 

R  15- 19,  at  first  I  lost  the  beats,  but  afterwards 
by  getting  li  15  well  into  action  before  R  19  was 
set  00,  and  keeping  on  pumping,  I  got  out  the  4  in 
a  second  quite  distinctly.  Hence  the  lowest  partial 
of  R  15  was  effective. 

R  15"  17,  here  also  I  once  heard  4  in  a  sec,  but 
this  must  have  been  from  the  Octaves. 

R  14*' 16,  I  was  quite  unable  to  distinsrnish 
anything  in  the  way  of  beats,  but  volleys  like  a 
feu  de  foie  about  a  second  in  length,  but  impossible 
to  count  accurately ;  thpy  may  have  been  2  in  a 
sec.  and  I  counted  double.  At  the  same  time  I 
seemed  occasionally  to  hear  a  low  beat,  so  low  and 
gentle  that  I  could  not  count  it,  and  the  great 
exertion  of  pumping  the  bel'ows  full  enough  to 
keep  these  two  low  reeds  in  action,  prevented 
accurate  observation. 

R  15  decidedly  seemed  flatter  thsn  R  i^,  so 
that  I  could  have  only  heard  the  lowest  partial  of 
R  15  and  the  Octave  of  R  13. 

On  soundinjj;  R  14  and  R  15  separately,  I 
seemed  to  hear  from  each  a  very  low  tone,  in 
quality  more  like  a  differential  tone  than  any- 
thing else.  This  could  also  be  heard  even  with 
R  13  and  R  Z2,  below  the  thumps,  and  even  in 
Rii. 

At  R  8  I  he^rd  only  the  sighing  of  the  escape 
of  wind  from  the  reed,  8  times  in  a  second,  as  well 
as  I  could  count,  and  I  also  heard  beats  evidently 
arising  ftom  the  higher  partials,  and  also  8  in  a 
.«iecond. 

At  R  9  there  was  the  same  kind  of  sishing  and 
equally  rapid  beats.  But  in  addition  I  seemed  to 
hear  a  faiui  low  tone. 


At  R  10  there  was  no  mistake  as  to  the  existence 
of  such  a  musical  tone. 

At  R  zi  and  R  12  it  was  still  more  distinct. 

At  R  13  the  tene  was  ven*  distinct  and  was 
quite  a  good  musical  tone  at  *R  14,  but  the  sish 
was  still  audible.  Was  this  the  lowest  partial  or 
its  Octave  ? 

R  16  gave  quite  an  organ  tone,  nothing  like 
a  hum  or  a  differentia],  but  the  sish  and  beats  . 
remain.    I  must  have  heard  the  lowest  partial,  ^ 
and  by  continual  pumping  I  was  able  to  Keep  it 
in  my  ear. 

R  x8  ••  20  gave  beats  of  2  in  a  see.  very  distinctly. 

Up  to  R  25  the  sish  could  be  heard  at  the 
commencement,  but  it  rapidly  disappeared.  It 
feels  as  if  the  tone  were  getting  gradually  into 
practice.  ITiis  effect  continued  up  to  R  22,  after 
which  the  sish  was  scarcely  brought  out  at  all. 
In  fact  long  before  this  the  sish  was  made  only  at 
the  first  moment,  and  was  rather  a  bubble  than  a 
sish. 

In  listening  to  the  very  low  beats,  the  beats  of 
the  lowest  partials  as  such  could  not  be  separated 
f^om  the  general  m&<<s  of  beats,  but  the  4  in  a  sec 
were  ^uite  clear  from  R  15-19.  The  lowest  pair 
in  which  I  was  distinctly  able  to  hear  the  bell-like 
beat  of  the  lowest  partials  distinct  from  the  general 
crash  was  R  30-34.  But  I  fancied  I  heard  it  at 
R  28-32. 

Prof.  Preyer  also,  in  the  same  place,  details  % 
his  experiments  with  two  enormous  tuning- 
forks  giving  13-7  and  18  6  vib.  The  former 
gave  no  musical  tone  at  all,  though  the  vibria- 
tions  were  visible  for  3  min.  and  were  dis- 
tinctly separable  by  touch.  The  latter  had 
*an  unmistakable  dull  tone,  without  droning 
or  jarring.'  He  concludes:  'Less  than  15 
vib.  in  a  sec.  give  no  musical  tone.  At  from 
16  to  24,  say  then  20  in  the  sec.  the  series  of 
aerial  impulses  begins  to  dissolve  into  a  tone, 
assuming  that  there  are  no  pauses  between 
them.  Above  24  begins  the  musical  character 
of  these  bass  tones.  Herr  Appunn,*  adds 
Prof.  Preyer, « informed  me  that  the  differen- 
tial tone  of  27-85  vib.,  generated  by  the  two 
forks  of  111-3  and  83-45  \ib.,  was  '*  surprisingly 
beautiful"  and  had  a  "wondrous  effect."' — 
Translator.] 


Digitized  by 


\^o0gle 


178  DEEP  AND  DEEPEST  TONES.  part  n. 

tinaous  and  musioal,  yet  for  aerial  vibrations  of  a  different  form,  for  example  when 
compound  tones  are  used,  discontinuous  pulses  of  sound  are  still  heard  even  witliin 
this  octave.  For  example,  blow  the  disc  of  the  siren  with  gradually  increasing 
speed.  At  first  only  pulses  of  air  are  heard ;  but  after  reaching  36  vibrations  in  a 
second,  weak  tones  sound  with  them,  which,  however,  are  at  first  upper  partials. 
As  the  velocity  increases  the  sensation  of  the  tones  becomes  continually  stronger, 
but  it  is  a  long  time  before  we  cease  to  perceive  the  discontinuous  pulses  of  air, 
although  these  tend  more  and  more  to  coalesce.  It  is  not  till  we  reach  1 10  or  1 1 7^ 
vibrations  in  a  second  (A  or  ^  of  the  great  octave)  that  the  tone  is  tolerably  con- 
tinuous. It  is  just  the  same  on  the  harmonium,  where,  in  the  cor  anglais  stop,  c 
with  132  vibrations  in  a  second  stiU  jars  a  little,  and  in  the  bassoon  stop  we  observe 
the  same  jarring  even  in  &  with  264  vibrations  in  a  second.  Generally  the  same 
observation  can  be  made  on  all  cutting,  snarling,  or  braying  tones,  which,  as  has 

^  been  already  mentioned,  are  always  provided  with  a  very  great  number  of  distinct 
upper  partial  tones. 

The  cause  of  this  phenomenon  must  be  looked  for  in  the  beats  produced  by  the 
high  upper  partials  of  such  compound  tones,  which  are  too  nearly  of  the  same  pitch. 
If  the  15  th  and  i6th  partials  of  a  compound  tone  are  still  audible,  they  form  the 
interval  of  a  Semitone,  and  naturally  produce  the  cutting  beats  of  this  dissonance. 
That  it  is  really  the  beats  of  these  tones  which  cause  the  roughness  of  the  whole 
compound  tone,  can  be  easily  felt  by  using  a  proper  resonator.  If  G,  is  struck, 
having  49^  vibrations  in  a  second,  the  15  th  partial  is  f'jj^,  the  i6th  g^\  and  the 
17th  ^"J  [nearly],  &c.  Now  when  I  apply  the  resonator  ^",  which  reinforces  ^" 
most,  and  f%  g"%  somewhat  less,  the  roughness  of  the  tone  becomes  extremely 
more  prominent,  and  exactly  resembles  the  piercing  jar  produced  when  f'^  and 
g"  are  themselves  sounded.  This  experiment  succeeds  on  the  pianoforte,  as  well 
as  on  both  stops  of  the  harmonium.    It  also  distinctly  succeeds  for  higher  pitches, 

f  as  far  as  the  resonators  reach.  I  possess  a  resonator  for  g'",  and  although  it  only 
slightly  reinforces  the  tone,  the  roughness  of  G,  with  99  vibrations  in  a  second, 
was  distinctly  increased  when  the  resonator  was  applied.* 

Even  the  8th  and  9th  partials  of  a  compound  tone,  which  are  a  whole  Tone 
apart,  cannot  but  produce  beats,  although  they  are  not  so  cutting  as  those  from  the 
higher  upper  partials.  But  the  reinforcement  by  resonators  does  not  now  succeed 
BO  well,  because  the  deeper  resonators  at  least  are  not  capable  of  simultaneously 
reinforcing  the  tones  which  differ  from  each  other  by  a  whole  Tone.  For  the 
higher  resonators,  where  the  reinforcement  is  slighter,  the  interval  between  the 
tones  capable  of  being  reinforced  is  greater,  and  thus  by  means  of  the  resonatoi^ 
g '  and  g'"  I  succeeded  in  increasing  the  roughness  of  the  tones  Gio  g  (having 
99  and  198  vibrations  in  a  second  respectively),  which  is  due  to  the  7th,  8th,  and 
9th  partial  tones  (/',  g",  a",  and  /",  g"\  a'"  respectively).  On  comparing  the 
tone  of  G  as  heard  in  the  resonators  with  the  tone  of  the  dissonances  f*  g" 

^  and  g"  a^'  as  struck  directly,  the  ear  felt  their  close  resemblance,  the  rapidity  of 
intermittence  being  nearly  the  same. 

Hence  there  can  no  longer  be  any  doubt  that  motions  of  the  air  corresponding 
to  deep  musical  tones  compounded  of  numerous  partials,  are  capable  of  exciting  at 
one  and  the  same  time  a  continuous  sensation  of  deep  tones  and  a  discontinuous 
sensation  of  high  tones,  and  become  rough  or  jarring  through  the  latter .f  Herein 
lies  the  explanation  of  the  fact  already  observed  in  examining  qualities  of  tone, 
that  compound  tones  with  many  high  upper  partials  are  cutting,  jarring,  or  bray- 
ing ;  and  also  of  the  fact  that  they  are  more  penetrating  and  cannot  readily  pass 
unobserved,  for  an  inteimittent  impression  excites  our  nervous  apparatus  much 
more  powerfully  than  a  continuous  one,  and  continually  forces  itself  afresh  on  our 

♦  [The  student  should  now  perform  the  ex-  punn's  Reed  pipes  of  3*2  and  64  vib.  in  the  South 

periments  on  the  Harmonical    indicated  on  Kensington  Museum.    Their  musical  character 

p.  22d,  note.—  Translator,]  is  quite  destroyed  by  the  loud  thumping  of  the 

t  [This  is  particularly  noticeable  on  Ap-  upper  partials. — Translator.] 


Digitized  by  VjOOQIC 


CHAPS.  IX.  X,  BEATS  OF   UPPEE  PAETIALS.*  179 

perception.*  On  the  other  hand  simple  tones,  or  compound  tones  which  have  only 
a  few  of  the  lower  upper  partials,  lying  at  wide  intervals  apart,  must  produce  per- 
fectly continuous  sensations  in  the  ear,  and  make  a  soffc  and  gentle  impression, 
without  much  energy,  even  when  they  are  in  reality  relatively  strong. 

We  have  not  yet  been  able  to  determine  the  upper  limit  of  the  number  of  inter- 
mittences  perceptible  in  a  second  for  high  notes,  and  have  only  drawn  attention  to 
their  becoming  more  difficult  to  perceive,  and  making  a  slighter  impression,  as  they 
became  more  iiumerous.  Hence  even  when  the  form  of  vibration,  that  is  the 
quality  of  tone,  remains  the  same,  while  the  pitch  is  increased,  the  quality  of  tone 
will  generally  appear  to  diminish  in  roughness.  The  part  of  the  scale  adjacent  to 
/  '"J,  for  which  the  ear  is  peculiarly  sensitive,  as  I  have  already  remarked  (p.  1 16a), 
must  be  particularly  important,  as  dissonant  upper  partials  which  lie  in  this  neigh- 
bourhood cannot  but  be  especially  prominent.  Now  f"%  is  the  8th  partial  of  f% 
with  366I  vibrations  in  a  second,  a  tone  belonging  to  the  upper  tones  of  a  man's  and  f 
the  lower  tones  of  a  woman's  voice,  and  it  is  the  i6th  partial  of  the  unaccented 
f^,  which  lies  in  the  middle  of  the  usual  compass  of  men's  voices.f  I  have  already 
mentioned  that  when  human  voices  are  strained  these  high  notes  are  often  heard 
sounding  with  them.  When  this  takes  place  in  the  deeper  tones  of  men's  voices, 
it  must  produce  cutting  dissonances,  and  in  fact,  as  I  have  already  observed,  when 
a  powerful  bass  voice  is  trumpeting  out  its  notes  in  full  strength,  the  high  upper 
partial  tones  in  the  four-times-accented  octave  are  heard,  in  quivering  tinkles 
(p.  1 1 6c).  Hence  jarring  and  braying  are  much  more  usual  and  more  powerful  in 
bass  than  in  higher  voices.  For  compound  tones  above  f%  the  dissonances  of  the 
higher  upper  partials  in  the  four-times-accented  octave,  are  not  so  strong  as  those 
of  a  whole  Tone,  and  as  they  occur  at  so  great  a  height  they  can  scarcely  be 
distinct  enough  to  be  clearly  sensible. 

In  this  way  we  can  explain  why  high  voices  have  in  general  a  pleasanter  tone, 
and  why  all  singers,  male  and  female,  consequently  strive  to  touch  high  notes.  If 
Moreover  in  the  upper  parts  of  the  scale  slight  errors  of  intonation  produce  muny 
more  beats  than  in  the  lower,  so  that  the  musical  feeling  for  pitch,  correctness,  and 
beauty  of  intervals  is  much  surer  for  high  than  low  notes. 

According  to  the  observations  of  Prof.  W.  Preyer  the  difference  in  the  qualities 
of  tone  of  tuning-forks  and  reeds  entirely  disappears  when  they  reach  a  height  of 
c''  4224,  doubtless  for  the  reason  he  assigns,  namely  that  the  upper  partials  of  the 
reeds  fall  in  the  seventh  and  eighth  accented  octave,  which  are  scarcely  audible. 


CHAPTER   X. 

BEATS  OF  THE  UPPER  PARTIAL  TONES.  If 

The  beats  hitherto  considered,  were  produced  by  two  simple  tones,  without  any 
intervention  of  upper  partial  or  combinational  tones.  Such  beats  could  only  arise 
when  the  two  given  tones  made  a  comparatively  small  interval  with  each  other. 
As  soon  as  the  interval  increased  even  to  a  minor  Third  the  beats  became  indistinct. 
Now  it  is  well  known  that  beats  can  also  arise  from  two  tones  which  make  a  much 
greater  interval  with  each  other,  and  we  shall  see  hereafter  that  these  beats  play 
a  principal  part  in  settling  the  consonant  intervals  of  our  musical  scales,  and  they 

*  [In  Prof.  Tyndall's  paper  *  On  the  Atmo-  throwing  the  horns  slightly  out  of  unison  ;  bat 

sphere  as  a  Vehicle  of  Sound,*  read  before  though  the  beats  rendered  the  sound  charac- 

the  Boyal  Society,  Feb.  12,  1874,  in  trying  the  teristic,  they  did  not  seem  to  augment  the 

distance  at  which  intense   sounds  could  be  range.'— Tmiu2ator.] 

heard  at  sea,  he  says  {Philosophical  Transac-  f  fOn  the  compass  of  voices  see  App.  XX. 

tions  for  1874,  vol.  clxiv.  p.  189), '  The  influence  sect.  N.  No.  I.— TrawsZator.] 
of  "beats"  was  tried  on  June   3   [1J57J]   by 


Digitized  by 


\^o(5gle 


i8o 


BEATS  OF  UPPER  PARTIALS. 


PART  II. 


must  consequently  be  closely  examined.  The  beats  heard  when  the  two  genera- 
ting tones  are  more  than  a  minor  Third  apart  in  the  scale,  arise  from  upper  partial 
and  combinational  tones.*  When  the  compound  tones  have  distinctly  audible  upper 
partials,  the  beats  resulting  from  them  are  generally  clearer  and  stronger  than 
those  due  to  the  combinational  tones,  and  it  is  much  more  easy  to  determine  their 
source.  Hence  we  begin  the  investigation  of  the  beats  occurring  in  wider  intervals 
with  those  which  arise  from  the  presence  of  upper  partial  tones.  It  must  not  be 
forgotten,  however,  that  beats  of  combinational  tones  are  much  more  general  than 
these,  as  they  occur  with  all  kinds  of  musical  tones,  both  simple  and  compound, 
whereas  of  course  those  due  to  upper  partial  tones  are  only  found  when  such  partials 
are  themselves  distinct.  But  since  all  tones  which  are  useful  for  musical  purposes 
are,  with  rare  exceptions,  richly  endowed  with  powerful  upper  partial  tones,  the 
beats  due  to  these  upper  partials  are  relatively  of  much  greater  practical  importance 
f^  than  those  due  to  the  weak  combinational  tones. 

When  two  compound  tones  are  sounded  at  the  same  time,  it  is  readily  seen, 
from  what  precedes,  that  beats  may  arise  whenever  any  two  upper  partial  tones'  lie 
sufficiently  near  to  each  other,  or  when  the  prime  of  one  tone  approaches  to  an  upper 
partial  of  the  other.  The  number  of  beats  is  of  course,  as  before,  the  difference  of 
the  vibrational  numbers  of  the  two  partial  tones  to  which  the  beats  are  due. 
When  this  difference  is  small,  and  the  beats  are  therefore  slow,  they  are  relatively 
most  distinct  to  hear  and  to  count  and  to  investigate,  precisely  as  for  beats  of  prime 
tones.  They  are  also  more  distinct  when  the  particular  partial  tones  which  gene- 
rate them  are  loudest.  Now,  for  the  tones  most  used  in  music,  partials  with  a  lovr 
ordinal  number  are  loudest,  because  the  intensity  of  partial  tones  usually  diminishes 
as  their  ordinal  number  increases. 

Let  us  begin,  then,  with  examples  like  the  following,  on  an  organ  in  its  princi- 
pal or  violin  stops,t  or  upon  an  harmonium  : 


^ 


i 


J. 


u    -i 


I 


=^3= 


zsiz 


^ 


^ 


H 


The  minims  in  these  examples  denote  the  prime  tones  of  the  notes  struck,  and 
the  crotchets  the  corresponding  upper  partial  tones.  If  the  octave  C  c  in  the  first 
example  is  tuned  accurately,  no  beats  will  be  heard.  But  if  the  upper  note  is 
changed  into  B  as  in  the  second  example,  or  e^  as  in  the  third,  we  obtain  the  same 
beats  as  we  should  from  the  two  tones  Bc,ot  c  d)^,  where  the  interval  is  a  Semitone. 
The  number  of  beats  (i6^  in  a  second)  is  the  same  in  each  case,  but  their  intensity 
is  naturally  less  in  the  foi-mer  case,  because  they  are  somewhat  smothered  by  the 
strong  deep  tone  C,  and  also  because  c,  the  second  partial  of  0,  has  generally  less 
force  than  its  prime,  j: 

In  examples  4  and  5  beats  will  be  heard  on  keyed  instruments  tuned  according 
to  the  usual  system  of  temperament.  If  the  tempered  intonation  is  exact  there 
will  be  one  beat  in  a  second,§  because  the  note  a"  on  the  instrument  does  not  exactly 


*  [But  as  upper  partial  and  combinational 
tones  are  both  simple,  it  is  always  simple  tones 
which  beat  together,  and  the  laws  of  Chap. 
VIII.  therefore  govern  all  beats.  With  a  little 
practice  the  bell-like  sound  of  the  beating  par- 
tials may  be  distinguished  amid  the  confused 
beating  of  harsh  reed  tones.  It  only  remains 
to  determine  when  and  how  these  extra  beating 
tones  arise. — TrarislatorJ] 

t  [See  p.  93,  notes  *  and  §.  On  English 
organs  the  open  diapason  and  keraulophon  or 
gamba  might  be  used. — Translator.] 

X  [On  tlie  Ilarmonical,  instead  of  varying 
the  Octave  in  C  c  by  a  Semitone  np  or  down, 


we  can  slightly  flatten  the  upper  note,  by  just 
pressing  it  down  enough  to  speak,  when  the 
beats  will  arise.  Or  by  using  the  d  and  d,  we 
can  produce  mistuned  Octaves  as  D  d^  or  I)j  d. 
And  for  the  Fifth  in  No.  4  and  5,  we  can  use 
d'  a"  or  d!  a\  or  take  this  mistuned  Fifth  lower, 
as  da'  or  d  a,  the  true  Fifth  being  <i,  a,  which 
may  be  contrasted  with  ii.—Translaior,] 

§  [Suppose  d'has  297,  then  equally  tempered 
a  ought  to  have  445  vibs.  The  third  partial  of 
d'  has  therefore  3  x  297 » 891  vib.,  and  the 
Octave  of  a  has  2x445  =  890  vib.,  and  these 
two  tones  beat  891  -890  =  once  in  a  second. — 
Translator.l 

Digitized  by  VjOOQlC 


CHAP.x.  BEATS  OF  UPPER  PARTIALS.  i8i 

tbgree  with  the  note  al\  which  is  the  third  partial  tone  of  the  note  dl.  On  the  otiier 
hand  the  note  a"  on  the  instrument  exactly  coincides  with  a!\  the  second  partial 
tone  of  the  note  al  in  the  fifth  example,  so  that  on  instruments  exactly  tuned  in 
any  temperament  the  two  examples  4  and  5  should  give  the  same  number  of  beats. 

Since  the  first  upper  partial  tone  makes  exactly  twice  as  many  vibrations  in  a 
second  as  its  prime,  the  c  on  the  instrument  in  Ex.  i ,  is  identical  with  the  first  upper 
partial  of  the  prime  tone  0,  provided  c  makes  twice  as  many  vibrations  in  a  second 
as  0*  The  two  notes  0,  c,  cannot  be  struck  together  without  producing  beats,  unless 
this  exact  relation  is  maintained.  The  least  deviation  from  this  exact  relation  is 
betrayed  by  beats.  In  the  fourth  example  the  beats  will  not  cease  till  we  tune  a" 
on  the  instrument  so  as  to  coincide  with  the  third  partial  tone  of  the  note  d,  and 
this  can  only  happen  when  the  pitch  number  of  a!'  is  precisely  three  times  that  of 
d'.  In  the  fifth  example  we  have  to  make  the  pitch  number  of  a!  half  as  great  as 
that  of  a",  which  is  three  times  that  of  d' ;  that  is  the  pitch  numbers  of  d'  and  a'  ^ 
must  be  exactly  as  2  :  3,  or  beats  will  ensue.  Any  deviation  from  this  ratio  will  be 
detected  at  once  by  beats. 

Now  we  have  already  shewn  that  the  pitch  numbers  of  two  tones  which  form 
an  Octave  are  in  the  ratio  1:2,  and  those  of  two  which  form  a  Fifth  in  that  of  2  : 3. 
These  ratios  were  discovered  long  ago  by  merely  following  the  judgment  of  the  ear 
respecting  the  most  pleasant  concord  of  two  tpnes.  The  circumstances  just  stated 
furnish  the  reason  why  these  intervals  when  tuned  according  to  these  simple  ratios 
of  numbers,  and  in  no  other  case,  will  produce  an  undisturbed  concord,  whereas 
very  small  deviations  from  this  mathematical  intonation  will  betray  themselves  by 
that  restless  fluctuation  of  tone  known  as  beats.  The  d'  and  a'  of  the  last  example, 
if  d'  tuned  as  a  perfect  Fifth  below  a  [that  is  as  dx  on  the  Harmonical],  make  293^ 
and  440  vibrations  in  a  second  respectively,  and  their  common  upper  partial  a!' 
makes  3  x  293^=2  x  440=880  vibrations  in  a  second.  In  the  tempered  intonation 
d!  makes  almost  exactly  293!  vibrations  in  a  second,  and  hence  its  second  upper  ^ 
partial  (or  third  partial)  tone  makes  881  vib.  in  the  same  time,  and  this  extremely 
small  difference  is  betrayed  to  the  ear  by  one  beat  in  a  second.  That  imperfect 
Octaves  and  Fifths  will  produce  beats,  was  a  fact  long  knOwn  to  organ-builders, 
who  made  use  of  it  practically  to  obtain  the  required  just  or  tempered  intonation 
with  greater  ease  and  certainty.  Indeed,  there  is  no  more  sensitive  means  of 
proving  the  correctness  of  intervals. 

Two  musical  tones,  therefore,  which  stand  in  the  relation  of  a^perfect  Octave, 
a  perfect  Twelfth,  or  a  perfect  Fifth,  go  on  sounding  uniformly  without  disturbance, 
and  are  thus  distinguished  from  the  next  adjacent  intervals,  imperfect  Octaves  and 
Fifths,  for  which  a  part  of  the  tone  breaks  up  into  distinct  pulses,  and  consequently  the 
two  tones  do  not  continue  to  sound  without  interruption.  For  this  reason  the  perfect 
Octave,  Twelfth,  and  Fifth  will  be  called  consonant  intervals  in  contradistinction  to 
the  next  adjacent  intervals,  which  are  termed  dissonant.  Although  these  names 
were  given  long  ago,  long  before  anything  was  known  about  upper  partial  tones  and  ^ 
their  beats,  they  give  a  very  correct  notion  of  the  essential  character  of  the  pheno- 
menon which  consists  in  the  undisturbed  or  disturbed  coexistence  of  sounds. 

Since  the  phenomena  just  described  form  the  essential  basis  for  the  construction 
of  normal  musical  intervals,  it  is  advisable  to  establish  them  experimentally  in  every 
possible  form. 

We  have  stated  that  the  beats  heard  are  the  beats  of  those  partial  tones  of  both 
compounds  which  nearly  coincide.  Now  it  is  not  always  very  easy  on  hearing  a 
Fifth  or  an  Octave  which  is  slightly  out  of  tune,  to  recognise  clearly  with  the  un- 
assisted ear  which  part  of  the  whole  sound  is  beating.  On  listening  we  are  apt 
to  feel  that  the  whole  sound  is  alternately  reinforced  and  weakened.  Yet  an  ear 
accustomed  to  distinguish  upper  partial  tones,  after  directing  its  attention  on  the 
common  upper  partials  concerned,  will  easily  hear  the  strong  beats  of  these  par- 
ticular tones,  and  recognise  the  continued  and  undisturbed  sound  of  the  primes. 
Strike  the  note  d',  attend  to  its  upper  partial  a",  and  then  strike  a  tempered  Fifth 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


i82  BEATS  OF  UPPER  PARTIALS.  partii. 

a' ;  the  beats  of  a!'  will  be  clearly  heard.  To  an  unpractised  ear  the  resonators 
already  described  will  be  of  great  assistance.  Apply  the  resonator  for  a'',  and  the 
above  beats  will  be  heard  with  great  distinctness.  If,  on  the  other  hand,  a  resonator, 
tuned  to  one  of  the  prime  tones  d!  or  a',  be  employed,  the  beats  are  heard  much  less 
distinctly,  because  the  continuous  part  of  the  tone  is  then  reinforced. 

This  last  remark  must  not  be  taken  to  mean  that  no  other  simple  tones  beat  in 
this  combination  except  a^'.  On  the  contrary,  there  are  other  higher  and  weaker 
upper  partials,  and  also  combinational  tones  which  beat,  as  we  shall  learn  in  the 
next  chapter,  and  tliese  beats  coexist  with  those  already  described.  But  the  beats 
of  the  lowest  common  upper  partials  are  the  most  prominent,  simply  bcksause  these 
beats  are  the  loudest  and  slowest  of  all.  • 

Secondly,  a  direct  experimental  proof  is  desirable  that  the  numerical  ratios  here 
deduced  from  the  pitch  numbers  are  really  those  which  give  no  beats.  This  proof 
f  is  most  easily  given  by  means  of  the  double  siren  (fig.  56,  p.  162).  Bet  the  discs 
in  revolution  and  open  the  series  of  8  holes  on  the  lower  and  16  on  the  upper,  thus 
obtaining  two  compound  tones  which  form  an  Octave.  They  continue  to  sound 
without  beats  as  long  as  the  upper  box  is  stationary.  But  directly  we  begin  to 
revolve  the  upper  box,  thus  slightly  sharpening  or  flattening  the  tone  of  the  upper 
disc,  beats  are  heard.  As  long  as  the  box  was  stationary,  the  ratio  of  the  pitch 
numbers  was  exactly  1:2,  because  exactly  8  pulses  of  air  escaped  on  one  rotation 
of  the  lower,  and  16  on  one  rotation  of  the  upper  disc.  By  diminishing  the  speed 
of  rotation  of  the  handle  this  ratio  may  be  altered  as  slightly  as  we  please,  but  how- 
ever slowly  we  turn  it,  if  it  move  at  all,  the  beats  are  heard,  which  shews  that  the 
interval  is  mistuned. 

Similarly  with  the  Fifth.  Open  the  series  of  12  holes  above,  and  18  below,  and 
a  perfectly  unbroken  Fifth  will  be  heard  as  long  as  the  upper  windbox  is  at  rest. 
The  ratio  of  the  vibrational  numbers,  fixed  by  the  holes  of  the  two  series,  is  exactly 
^  2  to  3.  On  rotating  the  windchest,  beats  are  heard.  We  have  seen  that  each 
revolution  of  the  handle  increases  or  diminishes  the  number  of  vibrations  of  the 
tone  due  to  the  12  holes  by  4  (p.  164c).  When  we  have  the  tone  of  12  holes  on  the 
lower  discs  also,  we  thus  obtain  4  beats.  But  with  the  Fifth  from  12  and  18  holes 
each  revolution  of  the  handle  gives  12  beats,  because  the  pitch  number  of  the 
third  partial  tone  increases  on  each  revolution  of  the  handle  by  3x4=12,  when 
that  of  the  prime  tone  increases  by  4,  and  we  are  now  concerned  witii  the  beats 
of  this  partial  tone. 

In  these  investigations  the  siren  has  the  great  advantage  over  all  other  musical 
instruments,  of  having  its  intervals  tuned  according  to  their  simple  numerical  rela- 
tions with  mechanical  certainty  by  the  method  of  constructing  the  instrument,  and 
we  are  consequently  relieved  from  the  extremely  laborious  and  difficult  measure- 
ments of  the  pitch  numbers  which  would  have  to  precede  the  proof  of  our  law  on 
any  other  musical  instnmient.  Yet  the  law  had  been  already  established  by  such 
^  measurements,  and  the  ratios  were  shewn  to  approximate  more  and  more  closely  to 
those  of  the  simple  numbers,  as  the  degree  of  perfection  increased,  to  which  the 
methods  of  measuring  numbers  of  vibrations  and  tuning  perfectly  had  been  brought 

Just  as  the  coincidences  of  the  two  first  upper  partial  tones  led  us  to  the  natural 
consonances  of  the  Octave  and  Fifth,  the  coincidences  of  higher  upper  partials 
would  lead  us  to  a  further  series  of  natural  consonances.  But  it  must  be  remarked 
that  in  the  same  proportion  that  these  higher  upper  partials  become  weaker,  the 
less  perceptible  become  the  beats  by  which  the  imperfect  are  distinguished  from 
the  perfect  intervals,  and  the  error  of  tuning  is  shewn.  Hence  the  delimitation  of 
those  intervals  which  depend  upon  coincidences  of  the  higher  upper  partials  be- 
comes continually  more  indistinct  and  indeterminate  as  the  upper  partials  involved 
are  higher  in  order.  In  the  following  table  the  first  horizontal  line  and  first  ver- 
tical column  contain  the  ordinal  numbers  of  the  coincident  upper  partial  tones, 
and  at  their  intersection  will  be  found  the  name  of  the  con-esponding  interval 
between  the  prime  tones,  and  the  ratio  of  the  vibrational  numbers  of  the  tones 

Digitized  by^OOQlC 


CHAP.  X. 


BEATS  OF  UPPER  PAKTIALS. 


183 


composing  it.    This  numerical  ratio  always  results  from  the  ordinal  numbers  of  the 
two  coincident  upper  partial  tones. 


Ooincideibt 
Partial  Tones 

I 

a 

3 

4 

5 

6 

2  Octaves 

and  Fifth 

I  :6 

Twelfth 
1:3 

Octave 

I  :  2 

Fifth 
2:3 

Minor 
Third 
5:6 

5         ] 

2  Octaves  A 

Major  Third 

1:5 

Major 
Tenth 
2:5 

Major 
Sixth 
3:5 

Major 
Third 
4:5 

4          { 

Donble  Octave 
I  :4 

Octave 
I  ;  2 

Fourth 
3:4 

3         { 

Twelfth 
I  :3 

Fifth 
2:3 

• 

2         1 

Octave 
I  :  2 

The  two  lowest  lines  of  this  table  contain  the  intervals  already  considered,  the 
Octave,  Twelfth,  and  Fifth.  In  the  third  line  from  the  bottom  the  4th  partial 
gives  the  intervals  of  the  Fourth  and  double  Octave.  The  5th  partial  determines 
the  major  Third,  either  simple  or  increased  by  one  or  two  Octaves,  and  the  major 
Sixth.  The  6th  partial  introduces  the  minor  Third  in  addition.  Here  I  have 
stopped,  because  the  7th  partial  tone  is  entirely  eliminated,  or  at  least  much 
weakened,  on  instruments  such  as  the  piano,  where  the  quality  of  tone  can  be 
reg^llated  within  certain  limits.*  Even  the  6th  partial  is  generally  very  weak,  but 
an  endeavour  is  made  to  favour  all  the  partials  up  to  the  5th.  We  shall  return 
hereafter  to  the  intervals  characterised  by  the  7th  partial,  and  to  the  minor  Sixth, 
which  is  determined  by  the  8th.  The  following  is  the  order  of  the  consonant ' 
intervals  beginning  with  those  distinctly  characterised,  and  then  proceeding  to 
those  which  have  their  limits  somewhat  blurred,  so  to  speak,  by  the  weaker  beats 
of  the  higher  upper  partial  tones  : — 

1.  Octave 1:2 

2.  Twelfth I 

3.  Fifth 2 

4.  Fourth 3 

5.  Major  Sixth 3 

6.  Major  Third 4 

7.  Minor  Third 5 

The  following  examples  in  musical  notation  shew  the  coincidences  of  the  upper 
partials.     The  primes  are  as  before  represented  by  minims,  and  the  upper  partials 
by  crotchets.    The  series  of  upper  partials  is  continued  up  to  the  common  tone  IT 
only. 


T 

Octave. 
I  :  2 


Twelfth. 

I  '  3 


Fifth. 
2  :  3 


p— T— pr-1 p- 

Fourth.  Maj.  Sixth.  Maj.  Third.  Min.  Third, 
3:4  3:5  4:5  5:6 


We  have  hitherto  confined  our  attention  to  beats  arising  from  intervals  which 
differ  but  slightly  from  those  of  perfect  consonances.     When  the  difference  is 

*  [But  see  Mr.  Hipkins'  remarks  and  experiments,  supra,  p.  'J^CJ  note.— 2><xiwto/or.] 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQ IC 


1 84 


BEATS  OF  UPPER  PARTIALS. 


PABT  II. 


small  the  beats  are  slow,  and  hence  easy  both  to  observe  and  count.  Of  course 
beats  continue  to  occur  when  the  deviation  of  the  two  coincident  upper  partials 
increases.  But  as  the  beats  then  become  more  numerous  the  overwhelming  mass 
of  sound  of  the  louder  primes  conceals  their  real  character  more  easily  than  the 
quicker  beats  of  dissonant  primes  themselves.  These  more  rapid  beats  give  a 
rough  effect  to  the  whole  mass  of  sound,  but  the  ear  does  not  readily  recognise  its 
cause,  unless  the  experiments  have  been  conducted  by  gradually  increasing  the 
imperfection  of  an  harmonic  interval,  so  as  to  make  the  beats  gradually  more  and 
more  rapid,  thus  leading  the  observer  to  mark  the  intermediate  steps  between  the 
numerable  rapid  beats  on  the  one  hand,  and  the  roughness  of  a  dissonance  oa 
the  other,  and  hence  to  convince  liimself  that  the  two  phenomena  differ  only  in 
degree. 

In  the  experiments  with  pairs  of  simple  tones  we  saw  that  the  distinctness  and 
^  roughness  of  their  beats  depended  partly  on  the  magnitude  of  the  interval  between 
the  beating  tones,  and  partly  upon  the  rapidity  of  the  beats  themselves,  so  that  for 
high  tones  this  increasing  rapidity  injured  the  distinctness  of  even  the  beats  arising 
from  small  intervals,  and  obliterated  them  in  sensation.  At  present,  as  we  have 
to  deal  with  beats  of  upper  partials,  which,  when  their  primes  lie  in  the  middle 
region,  principally  belong  to  the  higher  parts  of  the  scale,  the  rapidity  of  the  beats 
has  a  preponderating  influence  on  the  distinctness  of  their  definition. 

The  law  determining  the  number  of  beats  in  a  second  for  a  given  imperfection 
in  a  consonant  interval,  results  immediately  from  the  law  above  assigned  for  the 
beats  of  simple  tones.  When  two  simple  tones,  making  a  small  interval,  generate 
beats,  the  number  of  beats  in  a  second  is  the  difference  of  their  vibrational  numbers. 
Let  us  suppose,  by  way  of  example,  that  a  certain  prime  tone  has  the  pitch  number 
300.  The  pitch  numbers  of  the  primes  which  make  consonant  intervals  with  it, 
will  be  as  follows : — 

Hi 


Prime,  tone =300                                                                                 j 

Upper  Octave           =600 
„      Fifth             «450 
„      Fourth          =400 
„      Major  Sixth  =  500 
„      Major  Third  =375 
„      Minor  Third  =  360 

Lower  Octave           =150 
„      Fifth              =200 
„      Fourth           « 225 
„      Major  Sixth  =  180 
„      Major  Third  »  240 
„      Minor  Third  *  250 

Now  assume  that  the  prime  tone  has  been  put  out  of  tune  by  one  vibration  in 
a  second,  so  that  its  pitch  number  becomes  301,  then  calculating  the  vibrational 
immber  of  the  coincident  upper  partial  tones,  and  taking  their  difference,  we  find 
the  number  of  beats  thus : — 


Interval  upwards 


Beating  Partial  Tones 


XanilH»r  of  ' 
Beats 


Prime 
Octave    . 
Fifth       . 
Fourth    . 
Major  Sixth 
Major  Third 
Minor  Third 


300  « 
600  = 

450  = 
400  « 
500  = 

375  = 
360  = 


300 
600 
900 
1200 
1500 
1500 
1800 


301  = 

301  = 

301  = 

301 

301 

301 

301 


301 
602 

=  903 
=  1204 

=  1505 
«  1505 
=  1806 


Interral  downwariLi 


Prime 
Octave    . 
Fifth       . 
Fourth    . 
Major  Sixth 
Major  Third 
Minor  Third 


Beating  Partial  Tones 

Number  of 
Boats 

I   X   300  = 

300 

I   X  301  =     301 

I 

2  X   150  = 

300 

I    X  301  =     301 

I 

3   X   200  = 

600 

2   X   301   «     602 

2 

4   X    225  = 

900 

3  X  301  =    903 

3 

5  X  180  = 

900 

3  X  301  =    903 

3 

5  X  240  = 

1200 

4  X  301  =  1204 

4 

6  y  250  ^ 

1500 

5  ^   301       1 50s 

5 

Digitized  by  V^OOQIC 


CHAP.  X. 


BEATS  OF  UPPER  PARTIALS. 


185 


Hence  the  number  of  beats  which  arise  from  putting  one  of  the  generating 
tones  out  of  tune  to  the  amount  of  one  vibration  in  a  second,  is  always  given  by 
the  two  numbers  which  define  the  interval.  The  smaller  number  gives  the  number 
of  beats  which  arise  from  increasing  the  pitch  number  of  the  upper  tone  by  i. 
The  larger  number  gives  the  number  of  beats  which  arise  from  increasing  the 
pitch  number  of  the  lower  tone  by  i.  Hence  if  we  take  the  major  Sixth  c  a, 
having  the  ratio  3:5,  and  sharpen  a  so  as  to  make  one  additional  vibration  in  a 
second,  we  shall  have  3  beats  in  a  second ;  but  if  we  sharpen  c  so  as  to  make  one 
more  vibration  in  a  second,  we  obtain  5  beats  in  a  second,  and  so  on. 

Our  calculation  and  the  rule  based  on  it  shew  that  if  the  amount  by  which  one 
of  the  tones  is  put  out  of  tune  remains  constant,  the  number  of  the  beats  increases 
according  as  the  interval  is  expressed  in  larger  numbers.  Hence  for  Sixths  and 
Thirds  the  pitKsh  numbers  of  the  tones  must  be  much  more  nearly  in  the  normal  • 
ratio,  if  we  wish  to  avoid  slow  beats,  than  for  Octaves  and  Unisons.  On  the  other  % 
hand  a  sUght  imperfection  in  the  tuning  of  Thirds  brings  us  much  sooner  to  the 
hmit  where  the  beats  become  too  rapid  to  be  distinctly  separable.  If  we  change 
the  Unison  c"  c",  by  flattening  one  of  the  tones,  into  the  Semitone  b'  c",  on 
sounding  the  notes  together  there  results  a  clear  dissonance  with  33  beats,  the 
number  which,  as  before  observed,  seems  to  give  the  maximum  of  harshness. 
But  to  obtain  33  beats  from  fifth  /  c",  it  is  only  necessary  to  alter  c"  by  a  quarter 
of  a  Tone.  If  it  is  changed  by  a  Semitone,  so  that/  c"  becomes/  b',  there  result 
66  beats,  and  their  clearness  is  already  much  injured.  To  obtain  33  beats  the  c'^ 
must  not  be  changed  in  the  Fiffch  c"  g'^  by  more  than  one-sixth  of  a  Tone,  in  the 
Fourth  c"  /'  by  more  than  one-eighth,  in  the  major  Tliird  c"  e"  and  major  Sixth 
e'  a"  by  more  than  one-tenth,  and  in  the  minor  Third  c"  e"|>  by  more  than  one- 
twelfth.  Conversely,  if  in  each  of  these  intervals  the  pitch  number  of  c"  be 
altered  by  33,  so  that  c"  becomes  b'  or  d']},  we  obtain  the  following  numbers  of 
beats: —  .% 


The  Intcrra'  of  the 

becomes 

or 

anil  girea  beats 

Octave     .        .        .        .    d'd" 
Fifth       .        .        ,        .    d'g" 
Fourth    ....    cV" 
Major  Third    .        .        .    d' d' 
Minor  Third    .        .        .    d' d'b 

b'd" 
b'g" 
b'f" 
bfd' 
b'  d'\> 

d"\>  d" 
d")>  g" 
d"\>f" 
d"\>  d' 
d"\)  d'b 

66 

99 

132 

16S 

198 

Now  since  99  beats  in  a  second  produce  very  weak  effects  even  imder  favourable 
circumstances  for  simple  tones,  and  132  beats  in  a  second  seem  to  lie  at  the  hmit 
of  audibility,  we  must  not  be  surprised  if  such  numbers  of  beats,  produced  by  the 
weaker  upper  partials,  and  smothered  by  the  more  powerful  prime  tones,  no  longer 
produce  any  sensible  effect,  and  in  feict  vanish  so  far  as  the  ear  is  concerned.  Now 
this  relation  is  of  great  importance  in  the  practice  of  music,  for  in  the  table  it  will  ^ 
be  seen  that  the  mistuned  Fifth  gives  the  interval  h'  g",  which  is  much  used  as  an 
imperfect  consonance  under  the  name  of  mitior  Sixth,  In  the  same  way  we  find 
the  major  Third  d'^f  as  a  mistuned  Fourth,  and  the  Fourth  b'e"  as  a  mistuned 
major  Third,  and  so  on.  That,  at  least  in  this  part  of  the  scale,  the  major  Third 
does  not  produce  the  beats  of  a  mistuned  Fourth,  or  the  Fourth  those  of  a  mis- 
tuned major  Third,  is  explained  by  the  great  number  of  beats.  In  point  of  fact 
these  intervals  m  this  part  of  the  scale  give  a  perfectly  uninterrupted  sound,  with- 
out a  trace  of  beats  or  harshness,  when  they  are  tuned  perfectly. 

This  brings  us  to  the  investigation  of  those  circumstances  which  affect  the    I 
perfection  of  the  consonance  for  the  different  intervals.     A  consonance  has  been    • 
characterised  by  the  coincidence  of  two  of  tlie  upper  partial  tones  of  the  compounds    • 
forming  the  chord.     When  this  is  the  case  the  two  compound  tones  cannot  gene- 
rate any  slow  beats.     But  it  is  possible  that  some  other  two  upper  partial  tones  of 
these  two  compounds  may  be  so  nearly  of  the  same  pitch  that  they  can  generate 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQIC 


i86        DEGREE   OF  HARMONIOUSNESS  OF  CONSONANCES,    pakt  ir. 


rapid  beats.  Cases  of  this  kind  occur  in  the  last  examples  in  musical  notation 
(p.  183^).  Among  the  upper  partials  of  the  major  Third  FA  occur  f  and  e\  side 
by  sidjB ;  and  among  those  of  the  minor  Third  FA\}  will  be  found  a'  and  «'[>.  In 
each  case  there  is  the  dissonance  of  a  Semitone,  and  these  must  produce  the  same 
beats  as  if  they  had  been  given  directly  as  simple  prime  tones.  Now  although 
such  beats  can  produce  no  very  prominent  impression,  partly  on  account  of  their 
rapidity,  partly  on  account  of  the  weakness  of  the  tones  which  generate  them,  and 
partly  because  the  primes  and  other  partial  tones  are  sounding  on  at  the  same  time 
unintermittently,  yet  they  cannot  but  exert  some  effect  on  the  harmoniousness  of 
the  interval.  In  the  last  chapter  we  found  that  in  certain  qualities  of  tone,  where 
the  higher  upper  partials  are  strongly  developed,  sensible  dissonances  may  arifle 
within  a  single  compound  tone  (p.  1786).  When  two  such  musical  tones  are 
sounded  together,  there  will  be  not  only  the  dissonances  resulting  from  the  higher 
%  upper  partial  tones  in  each  individual  compound,  but  also  those  which  arise  from 
a  partial  tone  of  the  one  forming  a  dissonance  with  a  partial  tone  of  the  other,  and 
in  this  way  there  must  be  a  certain  increase  in  roughness. 

An  easy  method  of  finding  those  upper  partials  in  each  consonant  interval 
which  form  dissonances  with  each  other,  may  be  deduced  from  what  has  been  already 
stated  concerning  larger  imperfections  in  tuning  consonant  intervals  (p.  185c,  d). 
We  thus  found  that  the  major  Third  might  be  considered  as  a  mistuned  Fourth, 
and  the  Fourth  again  as  a  mistuned  Third.  On  raising  the  pitch  of  a  compound 
tone  by  a  Semitone,  we  raise  the  pitch  of  all  its  upper  partial  tones  by  the  same 
amount.  Those  upper  partials  which  coincide  for  the  interval  of  a  Fourth,  sepa- 
rate by  a  Semitone  when  by  altering  the  pitch  of  one  generating  tone  we  con- 
vert the  Fourth  into  a  major  Third,  and  similarly  those  which  coincide  for  the 
major  Third  differ  by  a  Semitone  for  the  Fourth,  as  will  appear  in  the  following 
example : — 


Major  Third. 


r 

WmoT  Third. 


The  4th  and  3rd  partial  in  the  Fourth  of  the  first  example  coincide  as/.  But 
if  the  Fourth  B[}  sinks,  as  in  the  second  example,  to  the  major  Third  A,  its  3rd 
partial/  sinks  also  to  «',  and  forms  a  dissonance  with  the  4th  partial/  of  F,  which 
was  unaltered.  On  the  other  hand  the  5th  and  4th  tone  of  the  two  compounds, 
which  in  the  first  example  formed  the  dissonance  a'  61?,  now  coincide  as  a'.  In 
'  the  same  way  the  consonant  unison  a'a^  of  the  second  example  appears  as  the  dis- 
sonance a'a'\}  in  the  third,  and  the  dissonance  c"c"^  in  the  second  becomes  the 
consonant  unison  c"c"  in  the  third. 

Hence  in  each  consonant  interval  those  upper  partials  form  a  dissonancey  tvhich 
coincide  in  one  of  the  adjacent  consonant  intervals,*  and  in  this  sense  we  can  say, 
that  every  consonance  is  disturbed  by  the  proximity  of  the  consonances  next 
adjoining  it  in  the  scale,  and  that  the  resulting  disturbance  is  the  greater,  the 

major  Third,  and  f  x^«=|  a  major  Tono. 
The  adjacency  of  the  consonant  intervals  is 
best  shewn  in  fig.  60,  A  (p.  193),  where  it 
appears  that  the  order  may  be  taken  as;  i) 
Unison,  2)  minor  Third,  3)  major  Third, 
4)  Fourth,  5)  Fifth,  6)  minor  Sixth,  7)  major 
Sixth,  8)  Octave.  In  the  table  on  p.  iSyft, 
other  intervals,  not  perfectly  consonant,  arc 
intercalated  among  these.  -  Translator.] 


♦  [That  is,  in  intervals  which  differ  from 
the  first  by  raising  or  depressing  one  of  its  tones 
by  a  Semitone  (either  |$  or  |J),  as  in  the  table 
on  p.  185c,  or  even  a  Tone  (?).  Thus  for  the 
Fifth,  3  X  If  =  5  a  minor  Sixth ;  and  §  x  g  =  J  a 
Fourth'  For  the  Fourth,  ^x|^r=?"a  major 
Third;  and  ^ivj^-^  a  Fifth!  For  the  major 
Tliird  i/xj';^^*  a  Fourth;  and  ^'^5!  =  ?  a 
minor  Third.    For  the  minor  Third  5  >  3k-^  2  a 


Digitized  by  VjOOQlC 


CHAP.  X.    DEGEEE   OF  HARMONlOUSNESS  OF  CONSONANCES.        187 


lower  and  londer  the  upper  partials  which  by  their  coincidence  characterise  the 
disturbing  interval,  or,  in  other  words,  the  smaller  the  number  which  expresses  the 
ratio  of  the  pitch  numbers. 

The  following  table  gives  a  general  view  of  this  influence  of  the  different  con- 
sonances on  each  other.  The  partials  are  given  up  to  the  9th  inclusive,  and  cor- 
responding names  assigned  to  the  intervals  arising  from  the  coincidence  of  the 
higher  upper  partial  tones.  The  third  column  contains  the  ratios  of  their  pitch 
numbers,  which  at  the  same  time  furnish  the  number  of  the  order  of  the  coincident 
partial  tones.  The  fourth  column  gives  the  distance  of  the  separate  intervals  from 
each  other,  and  the  last  a  measure  of  the  relative  strength  of  the  beats  resulting 
&om  the  mistuning  of  the  corresponding  interval,  reckoned  for  the  quality  of  tone 
of  the  violin.*  The  degree  to  which  any  interval  disturbs  the  adjacent  intervals, 
increases  with  this  last  number. f      ^  v^  ^  1  \  "?       I 


Interralfl 

Notation 

Ratio  of  the 
Pitch  Numbers 

Belative 
Distance 

Oants  In  the 

Intexrals 

Difference 
of  Cents 

Intensity 
of  Influence 

Unison 

c 

I  :  I 

8T9 

0 

204 

lOO'O 

Second 

D 

8:9 

63":  64 

204 

27 

14 

Snpersecond 

D  + 

7:8 

48 :  49 

231 

36 

1-8 

Subminor  Third 

Eb- 

6:7 

35:36 

267 

49 

24 

Minor  Third 

Eb 

5:6 

24:25 

316 

70 

3*3 

Major  Third 

E 

4:5 

35:36 

386 

49 

50 

Sapermajor  Third 

E  + 

7:9 

27T28 

435 

63 

1-6 

Fourth 

F 

3:4 

20 :  21 

498 

85 

,      8-3 

Sabminor  Fifth  . 

Gb- 

5:7 

14:  IS 

583 

119 

2-8 

Fifth  .... 

G 

2:3 

IS  :  16 

702 

112 

167 

Minor  Sixth 

Ab 

5:8 

24:25 

814 

70 

2-5 

1 

Major  Sixth 

A 

3:5 

20 :  21 

884 

85 

67 

Sabminor  Seventh 

Bb- 

4:7 

35:36 

969 

49 

3-6 

Minor  Seventh    . 

Bb 

5:9 

9 :  10 

1018 

182 

2-2 

Octave 

c 

I  :  2 

— 

1200 

— 

500 

The  most  perfect  chord  is  the  Umson,  for  which  both  compound  tones  have  the 
same  pitch.  All  its  partial  tones  coincide,  and  hence  no  dissonance  can  occur 
except  such  as  is  contained  in  each  compound  separately  (p.  1786).  II 

It  is  much  the  same  with  the  Octave.  All  the  partial  tones  of  the  higlier  note 
of  this  interval  coincide  with  the  evenly  numbered  partials  of  the  deeper,  and  re- 
inforce them,  so  that  in  this  case  also  there  can  be  no  dissonance  between  two  upper 
partial  tones,  except  such  as  already  exists,  in  a  weaker  form,  among  those  of  the 
/deeper  note.  A  note  accompanied  by  its  Octave  consequently  becomes  brighter 
Jn  quality,  because  the  higher  upper  partial  tones  on  which  brightness  of  quality 
depends,  are  partly  reinforced  by  the  additional  Octave.  But  a  similar  effect  would 
also  be  produced  by  simply  increasing  the  intensity  of  the  lower  note  without  add- 
ing the  Octave  ;  the  only  difference  would  be,  that  in  the  latter  case  the  reinforce- 
ment of  the  different  partial  tones  would  be  somewhat  differently  distributed. 

The  same  holds  for  the  Twelfth  and  double  Octave^  and  generally  for  all  those 


•  See  Appendix  XV. 

f  [Two  cohimns  have  bern  added,  shewing 
the  c^nts  in  the  intervals  named,  and  in  the 


intervals   between  adjacent  notes. 
App.  XX.  sect.  D. — TranslatoT.] 


See   also 


Digitized  by 


Google 


i88        DEGREE  OF  HABMONIOUSNESS  OF  CONSONANCES,    pabt  n. 

cases  in  which  the  prime  tone  of  the  higher  note  coincides  with  one  of  the  partial 
tones  of  the  lower  note,  although  as  the  interval  between  the  two  notes  increases 
the  difference  between  consonance  amd  dissonance  tends  towards  obliteration. 

The  cases  hitherto  considered,  where  the  prime  of  one  compound  tone  coincides 
with  one  of  the  partials  of  the  other,  may  be  termed  absolute  consonances.  The 
second  compound  tone  introduces  no  new  element,  but  merely  reinforces  a  part  of 
the  other. 

Unison  and  Octave  disturb  the  next  adjacent  intervals  considerably,  in  the  sense 
assigned  to  this  expression  on  p.  i86^,  so  that  the  minor  Second  C  jD\},  and  the 
major  Seventh  C  B,  which  differ  from  the  Unison  and  Octave  by  a  Semitone 
respectively,  are  the  harshest  dissonances  in  our  scale.  Even  the  major  Second 
C  D,  and  the  minor  Seventh  C  ^,  which  are  a  whole  Tone  apart  from  the  dis- 
turbing intervals,  must  be  reckoned  as  dissonances,  although,  owing  to  the  greater 
%  interval  of  the  dissonant  partial  tones,  they  are  much  milder  than  the  others.  In 
the  higher  regions  of  the  scale  their  roughness  is  materially  lessened  by  the 
increased  rapidity  of  the  beats.  Since  the  dissonance  of  the  minor  Seventh  is  due 
to  the  second  partial  tone,  which  in  most  musical  qualities  of  tone  is  much  weaker 
than  the  prime,  it  is  still  milder  than  that  of  the  major  Second,  and  hence  Ues  on 
the  very  boundary  between  dissonance  and  consonance. 

To  find  additional  good  consonances  we  must  consequently  go  .to  the  middle  of 
the  Octave,  and  the  first  we  meet  is  the  Fifth,  Immediately  next  to  it  within  the 
interval  of  a  Semitone  there  are  only  the  intervals  5  :  7  and  5  :  8  in  our  table,  and 
these  cannot  much  disturb  it,  because  in  all  the  better  kinds  of  musical  tones  the 
7th  and  8th  partials  are  either  very  weak  or  entirely  absent.  The  next  intervals 
with  stronger  upper  partials  are  the  Fourth  3  :  4  and  the  major  Sixth  3:5.  But 
here  the  interval  is  a  whole  Tone,  and  if  the  tones  i  and  2  of  the  interval  of  the 
Octave  could  produce  very  Uttle  disturbing  effect  in  the  minor  Seventh,  the  dis- 
%  turbance  by  the  tones  2  and  3,  or  by  the  vicinity  of  the  Fifth  to  the  Fourth  and 
major  Sixth  must  be  insignificant,  and  the  reaction  of  these  two  intervals  with  tlie 
tones  3  and  4  or  3  and  5  on  the  Fifth  must  be  entirely  neglected.  Hence  tlie  Fifth 
remains  a  perfect  consonance,  in  which  there  is  no  sensible  disturbance  of  closely 
adjacent  upper  partial  tones.  It  is  only  in  harsh  qualities  of  tone  (harmonium, 
double-bass,  violoncello,  reed  organ  pipes)  with  high  upper  partial  tones,  and  deep 
primes,  when  the  number  of  beats  is  small,  that  we  remark  that  the  Fifth  is  some- 
what rougher  than  the  Octave.*  Hence  the  Fifth  has  been  acknowledged  as  a 
consonance  from  the  earliest  times  and  by  all  musicians.  On  the  other  hand  the 
intervals  next  adjacent  to  the  Fifth  are  those  which  produce  the  harshest  disso- 
nances after  those  next  adjacent  to  the  Octave.    Of  the  dissonant  intervals  next 

*  [The  above  discuBBion  may  jbe  rendered  numbers  of  the  two  prime  tones  which  form 

easier  by  the  following  considerations,  which  the  Fifth  to  be  2  and  3,  and  find  those  of  their 

the   student  should  illustrate  or  hear  illus-  upper  pai'tials  thus,  assuming  C  6r  to  be  the 

m  trated  on  the  Harmonical.    Take  the  pitch  two  notes. 


Nos.  of  the  Partials 
Partials  of  lower  note 


Lower  note     . 

Fifth  or  2  :  3,  upper  note 


Partials  of  upper  note 
Nos.  of  the  Partials 


12345678 
2        4        6        8      10      12       14      16 


C        c        g        d      6'      (f      6'b 
Q  g  d!  ^  V 


3  6  9  12  IS 

12345 

We  see  that  the  principal  beating  tones  The  next  beating  partial  tones  arc  8  and  9,  or 

arc  14  and  15,  or  6'b  h\  the  7th  partial  of  the  c  d\  the  4th  partial  of  the  lower  and  3rd  of 

lower  and  5th  of  the  upper;  and  15  and  16,  the  upper  note,  and  these  being  a  whole  Tone 

or  b'  c'\  the  5th  of  the  upper  and  8th  of  the  apart,  the  beats  are  not  of  importance  even 

lower  note,  and  that  these  beats  are  unimpor-  when  strong,  and  with  weak  upper  partials 

taut  because  the  7th   and  8th  partials  are  are  insignificant.     Similarly  for  the  beats  of 

generally  weak ;  but  if  they  are  strong  these  9  and  10,  or  d'  e'y  the  3rd  partial  of  the  upper 

beats  being  those  of  a  Semitone  and  of  nearly  and  5th  of  the  lower  note.    On  referring  to  the 

a    Semitone,  arc  very  harsh.     On   the   Har-  text  it  will  be  seen  that  the  same  intervals 

monical  it  will  be  found  that  the  12th  G  g\^  are  there  compared  and  in  the  same  order  as 

faultless,  but  the  5th  C  G  is  decidedly  harsh.  here. — Translator.] 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


CHAP.  X.   DEGREE   OP  HARMONIOUSNESS  OF  CONSONANCES.        189 

the  Fifth,  those  in  which  the  Fifth  is  flattened,  that  is  which  lie  between  the  Fifth 
and  Fourth,  and  are  disturbed  firstly  by  the  tones  2  and  3,  and  secondly  by  the 
tones  3  and  4,  are  more  decidedly  dissonant  than  those  in  Which  the  Fifth  is 
sharpened  and  which  lie  between  the  Fifth  and  major  Sixth,  because  for  the  latter 
the  second  disturbance  arises  from  the  tone  3  and  the  weaker  tone  5.*  The 
intervals  between  the  Fifth  and  Fourth  are  consequently  always  considered  dis- 
sonant m  musical  practice.  But  between  the  Fifth  and  major  Sixth  lie^  the 
interval  of  the  minor  Sixths  which  is  treated  as  an  imperfect  consonance,  and  owes 
this  preference  mainly  to  its  being  the  inversion  of  the  major  Third.  On  keyed 
instruments,  as  the  piano,  the  same  keys  will  strike  notes  which  at  one  time 
represent  the  consonance  G  A\},  and  at  another  the  dissonance  C  Gjj^.f 

Next  to  the  Fifth  follow  the  consonances  of  the  Fourth  3  :  4  and  the  major 
Sixth,  the  chief  disturbance  of  which  arises  usually  from  the  Fifth.  The  Fourth 
is  somewhat  further  from  the  Fifth  (the  interval  is  8  :  9)  than  the  major  Sixth  is  ^ 
(the  interval  is  9  :  10),  and  hence  the  major  Sixth  is  a  less  perfect  consonance 
than  the  Fourth.  But  close  by  the  Fourth  lies  the  major  Third  with  the  4th  and 
5th  partials  coincident,  and  hence  when  these  partials  are  strongly  developed,  the 
Fourth  may  lose  its  advantage  over  the  major  Sixth.  It  is  also  well  known  that 
the  old  theoretical  musicians  long  disputed  as  to  whether  the  Fourth  should  be 
considered  consonant  or  dissonant.  The  precedence  given  to  the  Fourth  over  the 
major  Sixth  and  major  Third,  is  rather  due  to  its  being  the  inversion  of  the  Fifth 
than  to  its  own  inherent  harmoniousness.  The  Fourth,  the  major  Sixth  and 
minor  Sixth,  are  rendered  less  pleasant  by  being  widened  by  an  Octave  (thus 
becoming  the  Eleventh,  and  major  and  minor  Thirteenth),  because  they  then  lie 
near  the  Twelfth,  and  consequently  the  disturbance  by  the  characteristic  tones  of 
the  Twelfth  i  and  3,  is  greater,  and  hence  also  the  adjacent  intervals  2  :  5  for  the 
Eleventh,  and  2  :  7  for  the  Thirteenth,  are  more  disturbing  than  are  the  4  :  5  for 
the  Fourth  and  the  4  :  7  for  the  Sixth  in  the  lower  Octave.t  % 

*  [Taking  the  scheme  in  the  last  note,  and  supposing  O  to  be  altered  first  to  Ob  and  then 
to  ^b,  we  may  write  the  several  schemes  thus : 

No.  of  Partials  of  lower  note    ..12345678 


Lower  Note C        c  g  d       e  ^  h'\>     t 

Fifth  wi\z\  [     G  g  d'  g'            V 

Flattened       Worms  of  the  upper  note  sGb  gb  d'b  g'b          b'b 

Sharpened      )                                             I     Ab  ab  db  a'b          c" 


No.  of  Partials  of  upper  note    .        •  i  23  4  5 

If  the  Ob  were  made  sufficiently  flat,  we  3  of  the  upper  and  5  of  the  lower  note,  instead 
should  have  its  5th  partial  b'b  coinciding  with  of  from  d'b  c'  or  tones  3  of  the  upper  and  4  of 
the  7th  partial  of  C,  which,  however,  is  never  the  lower  note,  and  as  the  tone  5  is  weaker 
felt  as  a  consonance,  and  the  interval  then  the  disturbance  on  the  whole  is  weaker.    This 
becomes  5  :  7.    This,  however,  never  occurs  is  the  case  in  musical  practice. — Translator,] 
in  musical  practice,  where  the  b'b  from  Ob  is  f  [This  is  the  result  of  equal  temperament, 
always  sharper  than  that  from  C,  but  this  in  which  i4'b,  which  is  814  cents  above  C,  is 
dissonance  is  not  felt,  the  gb  g  or  tones  2  of  the  confounded  with  G^K  ,  which  is  only  772  cents  H 
upper  and  3  of  the  lower  note,  and  &  d'b  or  above  C,  a  difference  of  42  cents.     The  in- 
tones 3  of  the  upper  and  4  of  the  lower  note,  terval  c'  a^'b  can  be  played  on  the  Harmonical 
producing  the  chief    disturbance.    If  Ab  is  and  at  that  pitch  will  be  found  good.    The 
taken  sufficiently  sharp  for  its  5th  partial  cf'  interval  a*  b  e',  which  is  the  same  as  that  of 
to  coincide  with  the  eighth  of  C  we  have  the  dg'tU  1  but  a  major  Third  lower,  will  be  found 
interval  5  :  8  or  minor  Sixth.    Here  again  we  very  h&Tsh.—Translator.] 
have  the  disturbance  from  ab  g  the  tones  2  of  %  [Treating  these  intervals  as  in  the  pre- 
the  upper  and  3  of  the  lower  note,  but  the  ceding  notes  we  have : 
second  disturbance  is  now  from  db  0^  or  tones 

No.  of  Partials . 
Lower  note  « 
Fourth  or  3  :  4 
No.  of  Partials 


No.  of  Partials , 
Lower  note 
Elevmth  or  3  :  8 
No.  of  Partials 


12345678 
C       c        g        c'        €f       g^       b'b     c" 


F       f            d           f        a'  d' 

i_     2 3 4_    5 6_ 

2345678 
c        g        d        d       g'       6'b     c" 
/  f  c" 

I  2  3 


Digitized  by  VjOOQlC 


I90        DEGBEE   OF  HARMONIOUSNESS   OF  CONSONANCES,    part  ii. 


Next  in  the  order  of  the  consonances  come  the  major  and  miwyr  Third.  The 
latter  is  very  imperfectly  delimited  on  instmments  which,  like  the  pianoforte,  do 
not  strongly  develop  the  6th  partial  of  t^e  compound  tone,  because  it  can  then  be 
imperfectly  tuned  without  producing  sensible  beats.*  The  minor  Third  is  Bensibly 
exposed  to  disturbance  from  the  Unison,  and  the  major  Third  from  the  Fourth ; 
and  both  mutually  disturb  each  other,  the  minor  Third  coming  off  worse  than  the 
major.f  For  the  harmoniousness  of  either  interval  it  is  necessary  that  the  disturb- 
ing beats  should  be  very  rapid.  Hence  in  the  upper  part  of  the  scale  these  intervals 
are  pure  and  good,  but  in  the  lower  part  they  are  very  rough.  All  antiquity,  there- 
fore, refused  to  accept  Thirds  as  consonances.  It  was  not  till  the  time  of  Franco  <^ 
Cologne  (at  the  end  of  the  twelfth  century)  that  they  were  admitted  as  imperfect 
consonances,  Th^  reason  of  this  may  probably  be  that  musical  theory  was  developed 
among  classical  nations  and  in  medieval  times  principally  in  respect  to  men's 
voices,  and  in  the  lower  part  of  this  scale  Thirds  are  £bu:  from  good.  With  this  we 
must  connect  the  fact  that  the  proper  intonation  of  major  Thirds  was  not  dis- 
covered in  early  times,  and  that  the  Pythagorean  Third,  with  its  ratio  of  64  :  81, 
was  looked  upon  as  the  normal  form  till  towards  the  close  of  the  middle  ages^ 


No.  of  Partiah . 
Lower  note 
Major  Sixth  or  3  :  5 
No.  of  Partials . 

.     I         2 

.    C       c 
A 
I 

3 

9 
a 

2 

4 
c' 

5       6       7 
e!      g[       6'b 
e         a' 
3            4 

8 
5 

No.  of  Partials. 
Lower  note 

Major  Thi/rteenth  or  3  : 
No.  of  Partials. 

.     I      2 

.    C     c 
10 

3      4 

9      d 

a 

I 

5678 
</       ^       h'\>     c" 
a' 

2 

9 

10 

e' 
3 

No.  of  Partials. 
Lower  note 
Minor  Sixth  or  5  :  8 
No.  of  Partials . 

I        2 

.    C       c 
A\> 
I 

3 
9 
ab 

2 

4 

5        6        7 

«'       9"       6'b 
e'b             a'b 
3               4 

8 
c" 
c" 
5 

No.  of  Partials .        .        .  i     2    3    4  5      6      7  8      9      10      13       16 

Lower  note       .        .        ,  C   c    g    d  d     il     V\>  d'    dT     tf'      f      c" 
Minor  Thirteenth  or  s:i^                  a\>              a'b  «"b  a'b    c'" 

No.  of  Partials ...  i  2  345 


These  diagrams  will  make  the  text  imme- 
diately intelligible,  but  as  the  notes  refer  to 
the  ordinary  notation  the  fact  that  /  to  ^  in 
the  Fourth  is  a  wider  interval  than  g  to  a  in 
the  major  Sixth  is  not  expressed.  It  is,  how- 
ever, readily  seen  how  mach  worse  is  the 
minor  Sixth  with  g  to  ab,  and  that  in  all 
these  cases  the  disturbance  arises  from  the 
2nd  and  3rd  partials  which  coincide  for  the 
Fifth.  It  is  also  seen  how  the  disturbance  is 
increased  in  the  Eleventh  and  Thirteenths 
because  one  of    the    disturbing    tones    then 

No.  of  Partials .        .  .     i 

Lower  note       .        ,  .    C 

Major  Third  or  4  :  5  .        E 

No.  of  Partials .        .  •        i 


becomes  a  prime,  and  hence  Bounds  much  londcr. 
See  also  the  table  of  partials  on  p.  197c,  d.  - 
Translator.] 

*  [As  the  usual  tempered  tuning  of  the 
piano  makes  the  minor.  Third  greatly  too  flat, 
the  circumstance  mentioned  in  the  text  becomes 
a  great  advantage  on  that  instrument.  On 
the  tempered  harmonium  even  e'  ^,  e"  g*'  are 
very  harsh,  as  compared  with  the  same  inter- 
vals on  the  Harmonical. — Translator.'] 

t  [This  will  be  made  clearer  by  the  follow- 
ing  diagrams : 

2345678 
c        g        d        e'       g'       b'b     c" 

eh  e'  g%    h'        d" 

23  456 


No.  of  Partials . 
Lower  note 
Minor  Third  or  5 
No.  of  Partials . 


12345678 

C       c       g       d       e'       gT       h'b     d' 
Eb     eb      bb      db         g'  b'b      d"b 

12345  67 


The  6th  partial  of  this  Eb  is  not  the  same  as 
the  7th  partial  of  C,  although  the  notation 
makes  it  appear  so,  but  it  is  sharper  in  the 
ratio  of  36  :  35,  and  hence  if  the  partials  were 
not  so  high  would  be  very  disturbing.  It  is 
seen  that  g'  ^S  are  the  6th  and  5th  partials 
for  the  major  Third,  and  db  d  the  4th  and 
5th  for  the  minor  Third;  the  interval  being 
the  same  (24  :  25),  the  disturbance  is  worse  in 


the  latter  case,  because  the  partials  are  looser 
and  hence  louder. — Translator.] 

X  [The  ordinary  major  Third  on  the  tem- 
pered harmonium  is  very  little  flatter  than 
this,  but  still  it  is  much  less  harsh.  The 
Harmonical  does  not  contain  a  Pythagorean 
major  Third,  64  :  81,  the  nearest  approach 
being  !6b  :  rf,  -63  :  80,  but  it  contains  a  Py- 
thagorean minor  Third  df,  which  may  be  con- 


Digitized  by  V^jOOQIC 


CHAP.x.   DEGEEE   OF  HARMONIOUSNESS  OP  CONSONANCES.        191 

The  important  influence  exercised  on  the  harmoniousness  of  tlie  consonances, 
especially  the  less  perfect  ones,  by  the  rapidity  of  the  weak  beats  of  the  dissonajit 
upper  paurtials,  has  already  been  indicated.  If  we  place  all  the  intervals  alcove  the 
same  bass  note,  the  number  of  their  beats  in  a  second  varies  much,  and  is  much 
greater  for  the  imperfect  than  for  the  perfect  consonances.  But  we  can  give  all  the 
intervals  hitherto  considered  such  a  position  in  the  scale  that  the  number  of  their 
beats  in  a  second  should  be  the  same.  Since  we  have  found  that  33  beats  in  a 
second  produce  about  the  maximum  amount  of  roughness,  I  have  so  chosen  the 
position  of  the  intervals  in  the  following  examples  in  musical  notation,  as  to  give 


trasted  with  the  just  minor  Third  dj.  The 
following  arrangement  of  the  consonant  in- 
tervals will  show  the  beating  partials  in  each 
case,  and  the  exact  ratios  of  their  intervals. 
The  number  of  the  partial  is  subscribed  in 
each  case.  The  beating  interval  is  inoffensive 
for  5  :  6,  but  its  action  becomes  sensible  for 
7  :  8,  8  :  9,  and  9  :  10,  and  for  14  :  15, 15  :  16, 
24  :  25  the  effect  is  decidedly  bad  if  the  tones 
are  strong  enough  and  the  beats  slow  enough  ; 
the  strength  depends  on  the  lowness  of  the 


ordinal  numbers  of  the  beating  partials,  and  the 
rapidity  depends  on  their  position  in  the  scale. 
This  must  be  taken  into  consideration,  as  in 
fig.  60,  p.  193.  A  prefixed  *,  f ,  ^,  ||  draws  at- 
tention to  the  beatiog  partials.  The  order  of 
the  intervals  is  that  of  their  relative  harmoni-  % 
ousness  as  assigned  in  my  paper  *0n  the 
Physical  Constitution  and  Belations  of  Musical 
Chords,'  in  the  Proceedings  of  the  Boyal 
Society y  June  16,  1864,  vol.  xiii,  p.  392,  Table 
Vm.,  here  re-arranged. 


Cc 
Octave  or  I  :  2,  cents  1200 

CO 
Fifth  or  2  :  3,  cents  702 

Ce 
Major  Tenth  or  2  :  5,  cents  1386 


{■ 


4  s 

4> 


6    7 
6, 


8    9 
8, 


10 
10. 


{': 


Cg 
Twelfth  or  I  :  3,  cents  1902 

CF 
Fourth  or  3  :  4,  cents  498 

CA 
Major  Sixth  or  3  :  5,  cents  884 

CE  J 

Major  Third  or  4  :  5,  cents  386  \ 

CE\,  r  5, 

Minor  Third  or  5  :  6,  cents  316  \ 

cAb  ~r 

Minor  Sixth  or  5  :  8,  cents  814  \       8, 


r  2.    4,    6.  %  'lo. 

I        3,          6,        *9, 

12, 
12. 

ti4,    ti6, 
t»Si 

18. 

t20.. 

f  2,      4,      6,      84      10, 
•X                 Si                     10, 

12. 

•14,    *j6, 
•is. 

18, 

9 
9> 

20,, 
20, 

/  >        2       3       4       5 
I                    3, 

6 
6. 

7       8 

10 

6.    •9, 


12, 


ti5,      i8«      21, 
ti64     2O4 


24,    t2;,    t3o,, 

24a  J28, 


/  3i      6,    ♦9,      124 

I      5i  *io. 


«5s 
i5s 


4,      8,      12,    ♦id,      20, 
5i_    >o,_*i5, 20,_ 

10,      15,      20,    ♦25s 
6|        12,      18,    *244 


18.     t2lT      J24,        27,  30,, 

_t204 t25» 30.  _ 

t24.      28,      32,    t36,  40,0 

USs     30,     t35,  40. 


30.    t357    Uo*      45»    H50i» 
30*         t36fl     t42;    1148, 


/  5i      10* 


♦15.      20,    t25,    t3o.      35, 
*i6,    t24«  t32, 


40. 
40, 


Il45i.    llSo„ 


Ceb  f  Si    102    i5t    20,    ♦25,30.    t3S7    40,  t45 

Minor  Tenth  or  5  :  1 2, cents  1 5 16  \  12, 


*24, 


t36. 


tSOia  55ii  6o,j 
US,  60. 


Cf 


3i 


6,      ♦9. 

♦8, 


Eleventh  or  3  :  8,  cents  1698 

Ca  /  3i      6,      ♦9,      12,      15, 

Ma.Thrtnth.0r3  :  lo.cents  2084  \  ♦iOi 

Cab  /  5i     lOt      *i5«      204    25» 

Mi.  Thrtnth.  or  5  :  i6,cents  2014  (^  ♦id, 


ti5s      tiSe 
ti6. 


121, 


|24. 

i243 


t27.     30  • 


ti8.      Uh 

_    t20^| 

t3o/     t3S7 
t32. 


24s 


40. 


t27.     30  • 
t3o.    , 

USb         tSO;. 

t48. 


See  note  p.  195  for  the  intervals  depending  on  7. 


The  last  four  of  the  above  intervals  are  so 
rough  that  they  are  seldom  reckoned  as  con- 
sonances. The  order  was  determined  merely 
by  frequently  sounding  the  intervals  in  just 
intonation  on  justly  intoned  reed  instruments, 
and  relates  solely  to  the  effect  on  my  own  ear. 
The  greater  richness  of  the  major  Tenth  over 
the  Twelfth  made  me  prefer  the  former.  The 
effect  is  very  much  like  that  of  a  compound 
tone,  in  which  the  prime  is  inaudible ;  even 
the  tones  i  and  3  are  supplied  partly  by  com- 
binational tones.  Hence  when  a  man's  voice 
accompanies  a  woman's  at  a  Third  below  (that 
is  really  a  tenth)  the  cITect  is  moro  agreeable 


than  when  another  woman  sings  the  real 
Third  below,  as  long  as  the  Thirds  are  major ; 
the  contrary  is  the  case  when  the  Thirds  are 
minor.  In  ordinary  rules  for  harmony  no  dis« 
tinction  is  made  between  Tenths  and  Thirds, 
Fourths  and  Elevenths,  <&c.  The  above  table 
shews  that  the  differences  are  of  extreme  im- 
portance. The  dissonant  character  attributed 
to  the  Fourth  is  apparently  due  to  the  Eleventh. 
As  will  be  seen  hereafter,  the  minor  Tenth, 
the  Eleventh,  and  both  Thirteenths  ought  to 
be  avoided  or  else  treated  as  dissonances. — 
Translator.] 


Digitized  by.  VjOOQIC 


192        DEGREE  OP  HARMONIOUSNESS  OF  CONSONANCES,    part  ii. 

that  number  in  every  case.    The  intonation  is  supposed  to  be  that  of  the  scale  of 
C  major  with  just  intervals,  but  £{>  represents  the  subminor  Seventh  of  c  (4  :  7).* 


m. 


'^m 


=(=: 


:[:: 


8i:9i 


m= 


15,  :  16,     8,  :  15, 


=t=- 


Wz 


IP 


1 


6.  :  7,         S,  :  7, 


Eg"    I       ^ 


I 


42:7i 


Ss  :  Sa         57  :  6^ 


44=53 


33  :  53 


33  -4.2        2,  :  33 


The  prime  tones  of  the  notes  in  this  example  are  all  partials  of  (7^,  which 

makes  33  vibrations  in  a  second,  and  hence  their  own  pitch  numbers  and  those 

%  of  their  upper  partials  are  multiples  of  33  ;  consequently  the  difference  of  theso 

pitch  numbers,  which  gives  the  number  of  beats,  must  always  be  33,  66,  or  some 

higher  multiple  of  33. 

In  the  low  positions  here  assigned  the  beats  arising  from  the  dissonant  xtpper 
partials  are  as  effective  as  their  intensity  will  allow,  and  in  tliis  case  the  Sixths, 
Thirds,  and  even  the  Fourth  are  considerably  rough.  But  the  major  Sixth  and 
major  Third  shew  their  superiority  over  the  minor  Third  and  minor  Sixth,  by 
descending  lower  down  in  the  scale,  and  yet  sounding  somewhat  milder  than  the 
others.  It  is  also  a  well-known  practical  rule  among  musicians  to  avoid  these 
close  mtervals  in  low  positions,  when  soft  chords  are  required,  though  there  was 
no  justification  for  this  rule  in  any  previous  theory  of  chords. 

My  theory  of  hearing  by  means  of  the  sympathetic  vibration  of  elastic 
appendages  to  the  nerves,  would  allow  of  calculating  the  intensity  of  the  beats 
of  the  different  intervals,  when  the  intensity  of  the  upper  partials  in  the  corre- 
al sponding  quality  of  tone  belonging  to  the  instrument  used,  is  known,  and  the 
intervals  are  so  chosen  that  the  number  of  beats  in  a  second  is  the  same.  But 
such  a  calculation  would  be  very  different  for  different  qualities  of  tone,  and  holds 
only  for  such  a  particular  case  as  may  be  assumed. 

For  intervals  constructed  on  the  same  lower  note  a  new  f&cioT  comes  into  play, 
namely,  the  number  of  beats  which  occur  in  a  second ;  and  the  influence  of  this 
feustor  on  the  roughness  of  the  sensation  cannot  be  calculated  directly  by  any  fixed 
law.  But  to  obtain  a  general  graphical  representation  of  the  complicated  relations 
which  co-operate  to  produce  the  effect,  I  have  made  such  a  calculation,  k-nowing 
that  diagrams  teach  more  at  a  glance  than  the  most  complicated  descriptions,  and 
have  hence  constructed  figs.  60,  A  and  B  (p.  193).  In  order  to  construct  them 
I  have  been  forced  to  assume  a  somewhat  arbitrary  law  for  the  dependence 
of  roughness  upon  the  number  of  beats.  I  chose  for  this  purpose  the  simplest 
mathematical  formula  which  would  shew  that  the  roughness  vanishes  when  there 
f  are  no  beats,  increases  to  a  maximum  for  33  beats,  and  then  diminishes  as  the 
number  of  beats  increases.  Next  I  have  selected  the  quality  of  tone  on  the  violin 
in  order  to  calculate  the  intensity  and  roughness  of  the  beats  due  to  the  upper 
partials  taken  two  and  two  together,  and  from  the  final  results  I  have  constructed 
figs.  60,  A  and  B,  opposite.  The  base  lines  &&\  d'd"  denote  those  parts  of  tlie 
musical  scale  which  lie  between  the  notes  thus  named,  but  the  pitch  is  taken  to 
increase  continuously  [as  when  the  finger  slides  down  the  violin  string],  and  not  by 
separate  steps  [as  when  the  finger  stops  off  definite  lengths  of  the  violin  string]. 
It  is  further  assumed  that  the  notes  or  compound  tones  belonging  to  any  indi\ddual 
part  of  the  scale,  are  sounded  together  with  the  note  c\  which  forms  the  constant 
lower  note  of  all  the  intervals.  Fig.  60  A,  therefore,  shews  the  roughness  of  all 
intervals  which  are  less  thaji  an  Octave,  and  fig.  60  B  of  those  which  are  greater 


*■  [The  ordinal  nambers  of  the  partials 
which  beat  33  times  in  a  second,  are  here  sub- 
scribed.   Thus  4« :  5,  means  that  the  ratio  of 


the  primes  is  4:5,  and  that  the  beating  par- 
tials are  the  4th  of  4,  and  the  3rd  of  5,  having 
the  ratio  16  :  15.-  Traii&iaior.l 


Digitized  by  V^OOQIC 


CHAP.  X.    DEGREE   OF  HARMONIOUSNESS  OF  CONSONANCES.        193 

than  one  Octave,  and  less  tban  two.  Above  the  base  line  tbere  are  prominences 
marked  with  the  ordinal  numbers  of  the  partials.  Tbe  height  of  these  prominences 
at  every  point  of  their  width  is  made  proportional  to  the  roughness  produced  by 
the  two  partial  tones  denoted  by  the  numbers,  when  a  note  of  corresponding  pitch 
is  sounded  at  the  same  time  with  the  note  c'.  The  roughnesses  produced  by  the 
different  pairs  of  upper  partials  are  erected  one  over  the  other.*  It  will  be  seen 
that  the  various  roughnesses  arising  from  the  different  intervals  encroach  on  each 
other's  regions,  and  that  only  a  few  narrow  valleys  remain,  corresponding  to  the 
position  of  the  best  consonances,  in  which  the  roughness  of  the  chord  is  com- 
paratively small.  The  deepest  valleys  in  the  first  Octave  c'  c"  belong  to  the 
Octave  c\  and  the  Fifth  g' ;  then  comes  the  Fourth/,  the  major  Sixth  a\  and  the 
major  Third  «',  in  the  order  already  found  for  these  intervals.  The  minor  Third 
e't>,  and  the  minor  Sixth  a'^,  have  *  cols '  rather  than  valleys,  the  bottoms  of  their 

Fig.  60  A. 


IT 


Fig.  60  B. 


depressions  lie  so  high,  corresponding  to  the  greater  roughness  of  these  intervals. 
They  are  almost  the  same  as  for  the  intervals  involving  7,  as  4  :  7,  5  :  7,  6  :  y.f     ^ 

In  the  second  Octave  as  a  general  rule  all  those  intervals  of  the  first  Octave  are 
improved,  in  which  the  smaller  of  the  two  numbers  expressing  the  ratio  was  even ; 
thus  the  Twelfth  i  :  3  or  c'g'\  major  Tenth  2  :  5  or  c'e",  subminor  Fourteenth  2  :  7 
or  c'&"|>— ,  and  subminor  Tenth  3  :  7  or  c'e"l>— J  are  smoother  than  the  Fifth  2  :  3 
or  c'g\  major  Third  4  :  5  or  c'e',  subminor  Seventh  4  :  7  or  c'h'\)—y  and  subminor 
Third  6:  'jOTc'e'\}—.  The  other  intervals  are  relatively  deteriorated.  The  Eleventh 
or  c'f^  or  increased  Fourth  is  distinctly  worse  than  the  major  Tenth  or  &e" ;  the 
major  Thirteenth  or  c'a",  or  increased  major  Sixth,  is  similarly  worse  than  the 
subminor  Fourteenth  c'6"|7— .  The  minor  Third  or  c'e'\}j  when  increased  to  a 
minor  Tenth  or  c'e^y,t  ancl  the  minor  Sixth  or  c'a'b,  when  increased  to  a  minor 

♦  [The  method  in  which  these  diagrams 
were  calculated  is  shewn  in  the  latter  part  of 
Appendix  XV. — Translator.] 

t  [The  interval  4  :  7  is  over  6' b  —  ,  meaning 


'6'b  ;  the  interval  5  :  7  is  the  •  col '  between  / 
and  ^,  and  the  interval  6  :  7  is  the  next  *  col ' 
to  the  left  of  e'b.— Translator,] 

X   [By   carrying  a  line  down   from  e'b  in 


Digitized  by  V^OOQIC 


194        DEGREE   OF  HABMONIOUSNESS  OF  CONSONANCES,    part  ii. 

Thirteenth  or  c'a"\},  fare  still  worse,  on  account  of  the  increased  disturbance  of 
the  adjacent  intervals.  The  conclusions  here  drawn  from  calculation  are  easily 
confirmed  by  experiments  on  justly  intoned  instruments.*  That  they  are  also 
attended  to  in  the  practice  of  musical  composition,  notwithstanding  the  theoretical 
assumption  that  the  nature  of  a  chord  is  not  changed  by  altering  the  pitch  of  any 
one  of  its  constituents  by  whole  octaves,  we  shall  see  further  on,  when  considering 
chords  and  their  inversions. 

It  has  already  been  mentioned  that  peculiarities  of  individual  quaUties  of  tone 
may  have  considerable  effect  in  altering  the  order  of  the  relative  harmoniousness 
of  the  intervals.  The  quality  of  tone  in  the  musical  instruments  now  in  use  has 
been  of  course  selected  and  altered  with  a  view  to  its  employment  in  harmonic  com- 
binations. The  preceding  investigation  of  the  qualities  of  tone  in  our  principal 
musical  instruments  has  shewn  that  in  what  are  considered  good  qualities  of  tone 
^  the  Octave  and  Twelfth  of  the  prime,  that  is  the  2nd  and  3rd  partial,  are  powerful, 
the  4th  and  5th  partial  have  only  moderate  strength,  and  the  higher  partiaJs 
rapidly  diminish  in  force.  Assuming  such  a  quality  of  tone,  the  results  of  this 
chapter  may  be  summed  up  as  follows. 

I  When  two  musical  tones  are  sounded  at  the  same  time,  their  united  sound  is 
generally  disturbed  by  the  beats  of  the  upper  partials,  so  that  a  greater  or  less  part 
of  the  whole  mass  of  sound  is  broken  up  into  pulses  of  tone,  and  the  joint  effect  is 
rough.     This  relation  is  called  Dissonance. 

But  there  are  certain  determinate  ratios  between  pitch  numbers,  for  which  this 
rule  suffers  an  exception,  and  either  no  beats  at  all  are  formed,  or  at  least  only 
such  as  have  so  little  intensity  that  they  produce  no  unpleasant  disturbance  of  the 
united  sound.     These  exceptional  cases  are  called  Consonances. 

'I.  The  most  perfect  consonances  are  those  that  have  been  here  called  absolute, 

in  which  the  prime  tone  of  one  of  the  combined  notes  coincides  with  some  partial 

4  tone  of  the  other.     To  this  group  belong  the  OctavCy  Twelfth,  and  double  Octave. 

2 .  Next  follow  the  Fifth  and  the  i?bi*r^/i,which  may  be  called  perfect  consonances^ 
because  they  may  be  used  in  all  parts  of  the  scale  without  any  important  disturb- 
ance of  harmoniousness.  The  Fourth  is  the  less  perfect  consonance  and  approaches 
those  of  the  next  group.  It  owes  its  superiority  in  musical  practice  simply  to  its 
being  the  defect  of  a  Fifth  from  an  Octave,  a  circumstance  to  which  we  shall  return 
in  a  later  chapter. 

3.  The  next  group  consists  of  the  major  Sixth  and  the  major  Third,  which 
may  be  called  medial  consonances.  The  old  writers  on  harmony  ponsidered  them 
as  imperfect  consonances.  In  lower  parts  of  the  scale  the  disturbance  of  the 
harmoniousness  is  very  sensible,  but  in  the  higher  positions  it  disappears,  because 
the  beats  are  too  rapid  to  be  sensible.  But  each,  in  good  musical  qualities  of  tone, 
is  independently  characterised,  by  the  fact  that  any  little  defect  in  its  intonation 
produces  sensible  beats  of  the  upper  partials,  and  consequently  each  interval  is 

II  sharply  separated  from  all  adjacent  intervals. 

4.  The  imperfect  consonances,  consisting  of  the  minor  Third  and  minor  Sixths 
are  not  in  general  independently  characterised,  because  in  good  musical  qualities  of 
tone  the  partials  on  which  their  definition  depends  are  often  not  found  for  the 
minor  Third,  and  are  generally  absent  for  the  minor  Sixth,  so  that  small  imper- 
fections in  the  intonation  of  these  intervals  do  not  necessarily  produce  beats.f 

fig.  60  A,  it  will  be  seen  that  e"b  belongs  to  the  cVb  -and  cVb  — ,  the  student  should  take  the 

little  depression  to  the  right  of  the  fraction  J  same  intervals  a  Fourth  lower,  as  g  '6'b  and 

between  c"b- and  e".    The  slight  depression  g  ^bb.    All  the  other  notes  are  on  the  instm- 

for  a"b  is  just  under  the  fraction  ^^  to  the  left  ment  in  all  the  oct&ves.— Translator.] 

of  a".    The  depression  for  «'b-is  just  to  the  f  [It  must  be  recollected  that  in  the  minor 

left  of  that  for  e'b,— Translator.]  Sixth  the  2nd  and  3rd  partials  form  the  Seml- 

*  [The  student  is  strongly  recommended  tone   15  :  16,  and  the  3rd  and  5th  form   the 

to  verify  all  these  consonances  on  the  Har-  Semitone  24  :  2(;  (see  note  p.  191c),  and  that 

monical,  where  &b  — ,  that  is  '&b,  is  placed  on  the  resulting  beats,  which  in  good  qualities  of 

the  g\)  digital.    The  Harmonical  does  not  con-  tone  are   never  absent,  will  always  be  more 

tain  fib-,  that  is,  'eb,  and  hence,  in  place  of  powerful  than  those  which  arise  frcm  small 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


CHAP.  X.    DEGBEE  OF  HAKMONIOUSNESS  OF  CONSONANCES.        195 

They  are  all  less  suited  for  use  in  lower  parts  of  the  scale  than  the  others,  and 
they  owe  their  precedence  as  consonances  over  many  other  intervals  which  lie  on 
the  boundaries  of  consonance  and  dissonance,  essentially  to  their  being  indispens- 
able in  the  formation  of  chords,  because  they  are  defects  of  the  major  Sixth  and 
major  Third  from  the  Octave  or  Fifth.  The  subminor  Seventh  4  :  7  or  c'b]}—  is 
very  often  more  harmonious  than  the  minor  Sixtl  5  :  8  or  c'a'^,  in  fact  it  is 
always  so  when  the  third  partial  tone  of  the  note  is  strong  as  compared  with  the 
second,  because  then  the  Fifth  has  a  more  powerfully  disturbing  effect  on  the 
intervals  distant  from  it  by  a  Semitone,  than  the  Octave  on  the  subminor  Seventh, 
which  is  rather  more  than  a  whole  Tone  removed  from  it.*  But  this  subminor 
Seventh  when  combined  with  other  consonances  in  chords  produces  intervals  which 
are  all  worse  than  itself,  as  6  :  7,  5  :  7,  7  :  8,  &c.,  and  it  is  consequently  not  used 
as  a  consonance  in  modem  music.f 

5.  By  increasing  the  interval  by  an  Octave,  the  Fifth  c'g'  and  major  Third  ^ 
c'e'  are  improved  on  becoming  the  Twelfth  c'g'^  and  major  Tenth  cV.    But  the 

errors  of  intonation,  even  in  qualities  of  tone  *  [Reverting  to  the  diagrams  before  given 

in  which  an  8th  partial  is  well  developed. —  (p.  19 ic,  note),  we  may  compare  the  effect  of 
Trofulaton]  these  intervals  thns  : 

CAb  5i      io»    *i58      204      t255    t30«      357    40t    Il45.    Il50i« 

Minor  Sixth  or  5  :  8,  cents  814  8,  *i6,    f24j  1(324        40,  |i48« 

CjBb--C'£b  4i      *8,    t"«      164      $20,    ^24.      28,    32.      II36.    40,, 

Subminor  Seventh  or  4 :  7,  cents  969      "^7,  ti4j  ^21,  284        {I35, 

Hence  for  the  minor  Sixth  the  chief  beats  interval  which    replaces    the   15  :  16  in  the 

arise  from  the  interval   15  :  16,  or  the  3rd  minor  Sixth,  being  due  to  those  upper  partials 

partial  of  the  lower  and  2nd  of  the  upper  which  would  have   coincided   for  the  Fifth, 

note,  that  is,  from  those  tones  which  would  Both  CAb  and  C^B\>  can  be  played  on  the 

coincide  for  the  Fifth,  which  is  what  is  meant  Harmonical,  and  the  effect  in  the  different 

in  the  text  by  saying  that  the  interval  is  dis-  Octaves  should  be  compared. — Translator,] 
turbed  by  the  Fifth.     But  in  the  subminor  f  [In  fig.  60  A  (p.  193&),  the  bottom  of  the  ^ 

Seventh  the  chief  disturbance  is  from  7  :  8,  valley  of  4  :  7  above  2/b  — ,  is  just  a  little  lower 

or  the  prime  of  the  upper  and  2nd  partial  of  than  that  of  5:7,  between  /  and  ^,  and  than 

the  lower  note,  which  would  coincide  for  the  that  of  6:7,  which,  with  that  of  7  :  8,  lies 

Octave.    The  beats  from  the  interval  12  :  14  between  c^  and  0'b.    If  we  take  the  diagrams 

or  6  :  7  are  hardly  perceptible,  but  this  is  the  for  these  intervals  we  have : 

C^b-orG'Bb  6|       12,      18,    *244    t30.      tS^t    42,    §4«e       II 54.      6o„ 

Subminor  Third  or  6  :  7,  cents  267      7,        14,    ♦21,   f^S*      $35,  42^  §49,      IJ56, 

CGb-  or^-Bb  Si      10,    ♦15.    t204       2$^       30.    357      40^       45^      5oi« 

Subminor  Fifth  or  5  :  7,  cents  583       7,     ♦14,     f^ii  28,  35,  42,      49, 

CD+oT'BbC  7i       14.      21,      284     *35,      t42.    49*      S^t        63,      70,0 

Snpersecond  or  7  :  8,  cents  231  8,         16^      24,    ^324     f40,      48^  56,  64, 

The  second  forms  in  these  examples,  O  ^Bb,  almost  the  only  ones  noted  in  fig.  60  A.    In  the 

E  'Bb,  ^BbOj  can  be  played  on  the  Harmoni-  Supersecond  the  continual  repetition  of  the  in- 

eal.    We  see,  then,  that  6  :  7  is  disturbed  by  terval  7  :  8  produces  the  chief  effect,  but  32  :  35 

a  continual  repetition  of  this  same  intervid  from  the  4th  and  5th  partials,  and  40  :  42 

among  its  lower   partials,  and  also  by  the  =20  :  21,  from  the  5th  and  6th  partials,  also  f- 

intervals  21  :  24  »  7  :  8  from  the  3rd  and  4th  produce  much  effect,  as  shewn  in  the  fig.  60  A. 

partials,  28  :  30^  14  :  15  from  the  4th  and  5th  The  interval  7  :  9,  which  is  much  pleasanter,. 

partials,  and  35  :  36  from  the  5th  and  6th  has  not  been  considered  by  Prof.  Helmholtz,  but 

partials.    On  lookmg  at  the  diagram,  fig.  60  is  available  in  all  Octaves  on  the  Harmoni- 

A  (p.  193c),  it  will  be  seen  that  of  these  four  cal.    Mr.  Poole  distinguished  5  :  6,  6  :  7,  7  :  9 


the  first  is  chief,  but  the  others  are      as  the  minor,  minim,  and  maxim  Third,  here 

active.  For  the  subminor  Fifth  5  :  7  the  great  called  minor,  subminor,  and  super-major  Third, 
disturbance  is  from  14  :  15,  or  the  2nd  and  3rd  There  is  also  the  wide  (or  super)  minor  Third 
partial,  but  there  is  also  an  active  one  from  14  :  17.  I  add  the  anaJysis  of  the  two  last, 
20  :  21,  or  the  4th  and  3rd  partial,  and  these  are      both  of  which  are  on  the  Harmonical. 

'Bbd  7,       14,      2I3      *284      t35»      424      497      tS^n      63,. 

Bnper-major  Third  or  7  ^  9»  cents  435  9,  18,      *27,  t36|     45*      t54«      63, 

'b"b  »'d'"b  lii  282  42^  ^56;  tTol 

Super-minor  Third  or  14  :  17,  cents  336  17,  34^  ♦si,  1^84 

In  the  last  there  are  a  quantity  of  beating      result  is  really  superior  to  the  Pythagorean 
partials,  but  if  "d"'b  be  kept  as  here  high  in      minor  Third  27  :  32,  cents  2g4,— Translator.] 
the  sciJe,  they  will  not  be   heard,  and  the 


Digitized  by 


\^ot?gle 


196        DEGREE  OF  HABMONIOUSNESS  OP  CONSONANCES,    past  it 

Fourth  c'f  and  major  Sixth  c^d  become  worse  as  the  Elevenlii  df  and  major 
Thirteendi  da".  The  minor  Third  dd^  and  minor  Sixth  da!>^^  however,  become 
still  worse  as  the  minor  Tenth  dd'^  and  minor  Thirteenth  dd*^,  so  that  the  latter 
intervals  are  &r  less  harmonious  than  the  subminor  Tenth  3:7,  dd'^-—  [or  g  '^h*\f\, 
and  subminor  Fourteenth  2:7,  c'6"l>— . 

•  The  order  of  the  consonances  here  proposed  is  based  upon  a  consideration  of 
the  harmoniousness  of  each  individual  interval  independently  of  any  connection 
with  other  intervals,  and  consequently  without   any  regard  to  key,  scale,  and 

i  modulation.  Almost  all  writers  on  musical  theory  have  proposed  similar  orders 
for  the  consonances,  agreeing  in  their  general  features  with  each  other  and  with 
that  here  deduced  from  the  theory  of  beats.  Thus  all  put  the  Unison  and  Octave 
first,  as  the  most  perfect  of  all  consonances ;  and  next  in  order  comes  the  Fifth, 
after  which  the  Fourth  is  placed  by  those,  who  do  not  include  the  modulational 

%  properties  of  the  Fourth,  but  restrict  their  observation  to  the  independent  har- 
moniousness of  the  interval.  There  is  great  diversity,  on  the  other  hand,  in 
the  arrangement  of  the  Sixths  and  Thirds.  The  Greeks  and  Romans  did  not 
acknowledge  these  intervals  to  be  consonances  at  all,  perhaps  because  in  the  un- 
accented Octave,  within  which  their  music,  arranged  for  men*s  voices,  usually  lay, 
these  intervals  really  sound  badly,  and  perhaps  because  their  ear  was  too  sensitive 
to  endure  the  trifling  increase  of  roughness  generated  by  compound  tones  when 
sounded  together  in  Thirds  and  Sixths.  In  the  present  century,  the  Archbishop 
Chrysanthus  of  Dyrrhachium  declares  that  modem  Greeks  have  no  pleasure  in 
polyphonic  music,  and  consequently  he  disdains  to  enter  upon  it  in  his  book  on 
music,  and  refers  those  who  are  curious  to  know  its  rules,  to  the  writings  of  the 
West.*  Arabs  are  of  the  same  opinion  according  to  the  accounts  of  all  travellers. 
This  rule  remained  in  force  even  during  the  first  half  of  the  middle  ages,  when 
the  first  attempts  were  made  at  harmonies  for  two  voices.    It  was  not  till  towards 

f  the  end  of  the  twelfth  centmy  that  Franco  of  Cologne  included  the  Thirds  among 
the  consonances.    He  distinguishes : — 

1.  Perfect  Consonances :  Unison  and  Octave. 

2.  Medial  Consonances :  Fifth  and  Fourth. 

3.  Imperfect  Consonances  :  Major  and  minor  Thirds. 

4.  Imperfect  Dissonances  :  Major  and  minor  Sixth. 

5.  Perfect  Dissonances :  Minor  Second,  augmented  Fourth,  major  and  minor 
Seventh.f 

It  was  not  till  the  thirteenth  and  fourteenth  centuries  that  musicians  began  to 
include  the  Sixths  among  the  consonances.  Philipp  de  Vitry  and  Jean  de  Muris  t 
mention  as  perfect  consonances  the  Unison,  Octave,  and  Fifth ;  as  imperfect,  the 
Thirds  and  Sixths.  The  Fourth  has  been  cut  out.  The  first  author  opposes  the 
major  Third  and  major  Sixth,  as  more  perfect,  to  the  minor  Third  and  minor 
Sixth.  The  same  order  is  found  in  the  Dodecachordon  of  Glareanus,  1557, §  who 
If  merely  added  the  intervals  increased  by  an  Octave.  The  reason  why  the  Fourth 
was  not  admitted  as  either  a  perfect  or  an  imperfect  consonant,  must  be  looked  for 
in  the  rules  for  the  progression  of  parts.  Perfect  consonances  were  not  allowed 
to  follow  each  other  between  the  same  parts,  still  less  dissonances ;  but  imperfect 
consonances,  as  the  Thirds  and  Sixths,  were  permitted  to  do  so.  But  on  the  other 
hand  the  perfect  consonances.  Octaves,  and  Fifths  were  admitted  in  chords  on 
which  the  music  paused,  as  in  the  closing  chord.  Here,  however,  the  Fourth  of 
the  bass  could  not  occur  because  it  does  not  occur  in  the  triad  of  the  tonic.  Again 
a  succession  of  Fourths  for  two  voices  was  not  admitted,  as  the  Fourth  and  Fifth 
were  too  closely  related  for  such  a  purpose.    Hence  so  far  as  the  progression  of 

*  BtwpTtTiKhr  /i4ya  rris  Mowrucrit  vapii  Xpvtr-  1 852,  p.  49. 
dvBov,    Tfpy4trni,  1832,  cited  by  Coussemaker,  1  Coussemaker,  ibid.  p.  66  and  p.  68. 

Hiaiovre  de  Vharmoniej  p.  5.  §  [This  is  the  date    of  the   abstract  by 

f  Qerbert,  Scrij^Uyfes  acclesiastici  de  Mu-  Woneggar  of  Lithuania,  the  date  of  the  original 

•tea  Sacra,    Saint-lBlaise,  1784,  vol.  iii.  p.  11,  work  is  1547,  ten  years  earlier.— rmtwfcitor.]   . 
— Cousaemaker,  Eiitoir^  <fe  I'hamwnie,  Paris, 

Digitized  by  V^OOQIC 


eHAFS.  X.  XI. 


BEATS  DUE  TO  COMBINATIONAL  TONES. 


197 


iMuris  was  concerned,  the  Fonrth  shared  the  properties  of  dissonances,  and  it  was  at 
once  placed  among  them ;  bnt  it  would  have  been  better  to  have  placed  it  in  an  inter- 
mediate class  between  perfect  and  imperfect  consonances.  As  &r  as  harmonious- 
ness  is  concerned,  there  can  be  no  doubt  that,  for  most  qualities  of  tone,  the  Fourth 
is  much  superior  to  the  major  Third  and  major  Sixth,  and  beyond  all  doubt  better 
than  the  minor  Third  and  minor  Sixth.  But  the  Eleventh,  or  Fourth  increased 
by  an  Octave,  sounds  &r  from  well  when  the  third  partial  tone  is  in  any  degree 
strong.* 

The  dispute  as  to  the  consonance  or  dissonance  of  the  Fourth  has  been  con- 
tinued to  the  present  day.  As  late  as  1840,  in  Dehn's  treatise  on  harmony  we  find 
it  asserted  that  the  Fourth  must  be  treated  and  resolved  as  a  dissonance ;  but  Dehn 
certainly  puts  a  totally  different  interpretation  on  the  question  in  dispute  by  laying 
it  down  that  the  Fourth  of  any  bass  within  its  key  and  independently  of  the 
intervals  with  which  it  is  combined,  has  to  be  treated  as  a  dissonance.  Otherwise  ^ 
it  has  been  the  constant  custom  in  modem  music  to  allow  the  reduplication  of  the 
tonic  to  occur  as  the  Fourth  of  the  dominant  in  conjunction  with  the  dominant 
even  in  final  chords,  and  it  was  long  so  used  in  these  chords,  even  before  Thirds 
were  allowed  in  them,  and  in  this  way  it  came  to  be  recognised  as  one  of  the  superior 
consonances.t 


CHAPTEB  XL 

BEATS  DUE  TO  COMBINATIONiVL  TONUS. 

When  two  or  more  compound  tones  are  sounded  at  the  same  time  beats  may  arise 
£rom  the  combinational  tones  as  well  as  from  the  harmonic  upper  partials.  In 
Chapter  VII.  it  was  shewn  that  the  loudest  combinational  tone  resulting  from  two  ^ 

*   [See  the  Eleventh  analysed  in  p.  191c,  studied  on  the  Harmonioal,)  will  shew  generally 

iootDoie.— Translator,]  how  they  aflfect  each  other  in  any  combination. 

f  The  following  general  view  of  the  partials  The  nnmber  of  vibrations  of  each  partial  of 

of  the  first  16  harmonics  of  C  66,  (which,  with  each  harmonic  is  given,  whence  the  beats  can 

the  exception  of  the  nth  and  13th,  can  be  be  immediately  foand. 


Partials 
of  C 

s 
C 

3 

e 

3 

9 

'c 

f' 

6 
9' 

'^t> 

8 

S" 

xo 

0" 

zx 

X3 

9" 

43. 

'y'b 

J^' 

x6 
e"' 

I 

3 
4 
S 
6 
7 
3 

9 

lO' 

It 

13 

'3 

14 
15 
16 

17 
tS 
J9 

ao   ' 

44 

r    as 

26 

I  ^7 

^■ 

1  3'> 

33 

£6 

198 
a64 

330 
396 
462 
5^8 
594 
660 
736 
792 
BSB 
924 
990 
1056 

iiSS 

1^54 

1330 
i3j86 
1453 
1518 

isa4 
1650 
17 16 
lySa 
184S 
1980 

21 12 

13a 

396 
52S 
660 

792 
934 
1056 
118S 
1320 
1453 

198 
3^ 
594 

7Q2 

990 
1 188 
J386 
1SS4 

264 
528 
793 
1056 
1320 
1584 

330 
660 
990 
1320 
1650 

396 
792 
1 188 
1584 

463 
934 
1386 

528 
1056 

31 12 

594 
1188 
1783 

660 

1320 
1980 

736 

I5I8 

792 
1584 

858 
1716 

924 
1848 

990 
1980 

1056 

2IZ3 

Digitized  by  V^OOQIC 


J9^ 


BEATS  DUE  TQ  COMBINATIONAL  TONES. 


TART  ir. 


generating  tones  is  that  corresponding  to  tlie  difference  of  iheir  pitch  numbers,  or 
the  differential  tone  of  the  ffrst  order.  It  is  this  combinational  tone,  therefore, 
which  is  chiefly  -effective  in  producing  beats.  Even  this  loudest  combinational  tone 
is  somewhat  weak,  unless  the  generators  are  very  loud ;  the  differential  tones  of 
iiigher  orders,  and  the  summational  tones,  are  still  weaker.  Beats  due  to  such 
weak  tones  as  those  last  mentioned  cannot  be  observed  unless  all  other  beats  which 
would  disturb  the  observer  are  absent,  as,  for  instance,  in  sounding  two  simple 
tones,  which  are  entirely  free  from  upper  partials.  On  the  other  hand  the  beats  of 
the  flrst  differential  tones  [owing  to  difference  of  pitch  and  quahty]  can  be  heard 
very  well  at  the  same  time  as  those  due  to  the  harmonic  upper  partials  of  com- 
jpound  tones,  by  an  ear  accustomed  to  hear  combinational  tones. 

The  differential  tones  of  the  first  order  alone,  and  independently  of  the  com^ 
binational  tones  of  higher  orders,  are  capable  of  causing  beats  (i)  when  two 

^  compound  tones  sound  together,  (2)  when  three  or  more  simple  or  compound 
tones  sound  together.  On  the  other  hand  beats  generated  by  combinational  tones 
of  higher  orders  have  to  be  considered  when  two  simple  tones  sound  together. 

We  commence  with  the  differential  tones  of  compound  tones.  In  the  same 
way  that  the  prime  tones  in  such  cases  develop  combinational  tones,  any  pair  of 
upper  partials  of  the  two  compounds  will  also  develop  combinational  tones,  but 
such  tones  will  diminish  very  rapidly  in  intensity  as  the  upper  partials  become 
weaker.  When  one  or  more  of  these  combinational  tones  nearly  coincide  with 
other  combinational  tones,  or  the  primes  or  upper  partials  of  the  generators,  beats 
ensue.  Let  us  take  as  an  example  a  slightly  incorrectly  tuned  Fifth,  having  the 
pitch  numbers  200  and  301,  in  place  of  200  and  300,  as  in  a  justly  intoned  Fifth. 
We  calculate  the  vibrational  numbers  of  the  upper  partials  by  multiplying  those 
of  the  primes  by  i,  2,  3,  and  so  on.  We  find  the  vibrational  numbers  of  the  dif 
ferential  tones  of  the  first  order,  by  subtracting  these  numbers  from  each  other, 

f  two  and  two.  The  following  table  contains  in  the  first  horizontal  line  andvertical 
column  the  vibrational  numbers  of  the  several  partials  of  the  two  compound  tones, 
and  in  their  intersections  the  differences  6f  thbse  numbers,  which  are  the  pitch 
numbers  of  the  differential  tones  due  to  th^m. 


— 

Partials  of  the  Fifth 

• 

301 

602 

903 

"SS  f?^ 

lOI 

402 

703 

lil  1' 

400 

99 

202 

503 

)6oo 

299 

2 

303 

800 

499 

'^ 

103 

^  fl^ 

llDOO 

^99 

398 

97 

/ 

If  we  arrange  these  tones  by  pitch  we  find  the  following  groups : — 


97 

198 

299 

398 

499 

600 

699 

99 

200 

301 

400 

503 

602 

703 

lOI 

202 

303 

402 

103 

800      903 


1000 


The  number  2  is  too  small  to  correspond  to  a  combinational  tone.  It  only 
shews  the  number  of  beats  due  to  the  two  upper  partials  600  and  602.*  In  all  the 
Other  groups,  however,  tones  are  found  whose  vibrational  numbers  differ  by  2, 4,  or 
6,  and  hence  produce  respectively  2,  4,  and  6  beats  in  the  same  time  that  the  two 
first-named  partials  produce  2  beats.  The  two  strongest  combinational  tones  are 
loi  and  99,  and  these  also  are  well  distinguished  from  the  rest  by  their  low  pitch 

We  observe  in  this  example  that  the  slowest  beats  due  to  the  combinational 
tones  are  the  same  in  number  as  those  due  to  the  upper  partials  [600  and  602]. 
This  is  a  general  rule  and  appHes  to  all  intervals.f 


♦  [The  last  three,  800,  903,  1000,  are 
simply  non-beating  upper  partiaJB.  — ^Trans- 
lator.-] 


i  [But  the  beats  of  the  upper  partials  are 
always  distinguished  by  their  high  pitch.— 
Translator.] 


Digitized  by 


Google 


CHAP.  XI.  BEATS  DUE  TO  COMBINATIONAL  TONES.  199 

Further  it  is  easy  to  see  that  if  in  our  example  we  replaced  200  and  301,  by 
the  numbers  200  and  300  belonging  to  the  perfect  Fifth,  all  the  numbers  in  our 
table  would  become  multiples  of  100,  and  hence  all  the  different  combinational  and 
upper  partial  tones  wliich  now  beat  would  become  coincident  and  not  generate  any 
beats.  What  is  here  shewn  to  be  the  case  in  this  example  for  the  Fifth  is  also  true 
for  all  other  harmonic  intervals.* 

The  first  differential  tones  of  compounds  cannot  generate  beats,  except  when  the 
upper  partials  of  the  same  compounds  generate  them,  and  the  rapidity  of  the  beats 
is  the  same  in  both  cases,  supposing  that  the  series  of  upper  partials  is  complete. 
Hence  the  addition  of  combinational  tones  makes  no  essential  difference  in  the 
results  obtained  in  the  last  chapter  on  investigating  the  beats  due  to  the  upper 
partials  only.    There  can  be  only  a  slight  increase  in  the  strength  of  the  beats.f 

But  the  case  is  essentially  different  when  two  simple  tones  are  sounded  together, 
so  that  there  are  no  upper  partials  to  consider.  If  combinational  tones  were  not  f 
taken  into  account,  two  simple  tones,  as  those  of  tuning-forks  or  stopped  organ 
pipes,  could  not  produce  beats  unless  they  were  very  nearly  of  the  same  pitch,  and 
such  beats  are  strong  when  their  interval  is  a  minor  or  major  Second,  but  weak  for 
a  Third  and  then  only  recognisable  in  the  lower  parts  of  the  scale  (p.  iTid),  and 
they  gradually  diminish  in  distinctness  as  the  interval  increases,  without  shewing 
any  special  differences  for  the  harmonic  intervals  themselves.  For  any  larger 
interval  between  two  simple  tones  there  would  be  absolutely  no  beats  at  all,  if 
there  were  no  upper  partial  or  combinational  tones,  and  hence  the  consonant 
intervals  discovered  in  the  former  chapter  would  be  in  no  respect  distinguished 
from  adjacent  intervals ;  there  would  in  &ct  be  no  distinction  at  all  between  wide 
consonant  intervals  and  absolutely  dissonant  intervals* 

Now  such  wider  intervals  between  simple  tones  are  known  to  produce  beats, 
although  very  much  weaker  than  those  hitherto  considered,  so  that  even  for  such 
tones  there  is  a  difference  between  consonances  and  dissonances,  although  it  is  ^ 
very  much  more  imperfect  than  for  compound  tones.    And  these  facts  depend,  as 
Scheibler  shewed,}:  on  the  combinational  tones  of  higher  orders. 

It  is  only  for  the  Octave  that  the  first  differential  tone  suffices.  If  the  lower 
note  makes  100  vibrations  in  a  second,  while  the  imperfect  Octave  makes  201,  the 
first  differential  tone  makes  201  —  100=101,  and  hence  nearly  coincides  with  the 
lower  note  of  100  vibrations,  producing  one  beat  for  each  100  vibrations.  There 
is  no  difficulty  in  hearing  these  beats,  and  hence  it  is  easily  possible  to  distinguish 
imperfect  Octaves  from  perfect  ones,  even  for  simple  tones,  by  the  beats  produced 
by  the  former.§ 

For  the  Fifth,  the  first  order  of  differential  tones  no  longer  suffices.  Take  an 
imperfect  Fifth  with  the  ratio  200  :  301  ;  then  the  differential  tone  of  the  first 
order  is  loi,  which  is  too  far  from  either  primary  to  generate  beats.  But  it  forms 
an  imperfect  Octave  with  the  tone  200,  and,  as  just  seen,  in  such  a  case  beats  ensue. 
Here  they  are  produced  by  the  differential  tone  99  arising  from  the  tone  10 1  and  IT 

*  This  is  proved  mathematically  in  Ap-  phyaikalische  rmd    mtuikaUsche    Tonmesaer, 

pendiz  XVI.  (fee.) — Essen,  G.  D.  Badeker,  1834,  pp.  viiL  80, 

t  [The  great  difference  in  the  pitch  of  the  5  lithographed  Tables  (called  3  on  title-page)* 

two  sets  of  beats,  which  are  not  necessarily  and  an,  engraving  of  tuning-forks  and  waves, 

even  Octaves  of  each  other,  keeps  them  well  A   most  remarkable  pamphlet,  bat  unfortu- 

apart.    The  beating  partials,  in  this  case  600,  nately  very  obscurely  written,  as  the  author  says 

602,  and  the  beating  differentials,  here  loi  and  in  his  preface,  *  to  write  clearly  and  briefly  on  a 

99,  are  entirely  removed  from  each  other. —  scientific  subject  is  a  skill  (Fertigkeit)  I  do  not 

Translator,]  possess,  and  have  never  attempted.'    See  also 

X  [*  The  physical  and  musical  Tonometer,  App.  XX.  sect.  B.  No.  7.  I  do  not  find  any- 
-which  makes  evident  to  the  eye,  by  means  of  where  that  Scheibler  attempted  to  shew  that 
the  pendulum,  the  absolute  vibrations  of  the  combinational  tones  existed,  especially  inter- 
tones,  and  of  the  principal  kinds  of  combina^  mediate  ones ;  he  merely  assumed  them  and 
tional  tones,  as  well  as  the  most  precise  exact-  found  the  beats. — Translator,] 
ness  of  equally  tempered  and  mathematical  §  [See  App.  XX.  sect.  L.  art.  3,  latter  part 
chords,  invented  and  executed  by  Heinrich  oid. — Translator.] 
^>cheibler,  silk  manufacturer  in  Crefeld.'     {Der 


Digitized  by 


Google 


aoo  BEATS  DUE  TO  COMBINATIONAL  TONES.  pabt  a 

the  tone  200,  and  this  tone  99  makes  two  beats  in  a  second  with  the  tone  101. 
These  beats  then  serve  to  distinguish  the  imperfect  from  the  justly  intoned  Fifih 
even  in  the  case  of  two  simple  tones.  The  number  of  these  beats  is  also  exactly 
as  many  as  if  they  were  beats  due  to  the  upper  partial  tones.*  But  to  observe 
these  beats  the  two  primary  tones  must  be  loud,  and  the  ear  must  not  be  distracted 
by  any  extraneous  noise.  Under  favourable  circumstances,  however,  they  are  not 
difficult  to  hear.f 

For  an  imperfect  Fourth,  having,  say,  the  vibrational  numbers  300  and  401 » 
the  first  differential  tone  is  loi  ;  this  with  the  tone  300  produces  the  differential 
tone  199  of  the  second  order,  and  this  again  with  the  tone  401  the  differential  tone 
202  of  the  third  order,  and  this  makes  3  beats  with  the  differential  tone  199  of  the 
second  order,  that  is,  precisely  as  many  beats  as  would  have  been  generated  by  the 
upper  partial  tones  1200  and  1203,  if  they  had  existed.  These  beats  of  the  Fourth 
f  are  very  weak  even  when  the  primary  tones  are  powerful.  Perfect  quiet  and  great 
attention  are  necessary  for  observing  them.j:  And  after  all  there  may  be  a  doubt 
whether  by  strong  excitement  of  the  primary  tones,  weak  partials  may  not  have 
arisen,  as  we  already  considered  on  p.  1592^,  c,  § 

The  beats  of  an  imperfect  Tnajor  Third  are  scarcely  recognisable,  even  under  the 
most  favourable  conditions.  If  we  take  as  the  vibrational  numbers  of  the  primary 
tones  400  and  501,  we  have : — 

501—400=101,  the  differential  tone  of  the  first  order 
400— ioi:=299,  ,,  ,,       ,,      second  „ 

501—299=202,  „  „        „      third    „ 

400—202=198,  ,,       ,,      fourth  ,, 

The  tones  202  and  198  produce  4  oeats.  Scheibler  succeeded  in  counting  these 
beats  of  the  imperfect  major  Third.**    I  have  myself  believed  that  I  heard  them 

f  under  favourable  circumstances.  But  in  any  case  they  are  so  difficult  to  perceive 
that  they  are  not  of  any  importance  in  distinguishing  consonance  from  dissonance. 
Hence  it  foUows  that  two  simple  tones  making  various  intervals  adjacent  to  the 
major  Third  and  sounded  together  wiU  produce  a  uniform  uninterrupted  mass  of 
sound,  without  any  break  in  their  harmoniousness,  provided  that  they  do  not 
approach  a  Second  too  closely  on  the  one  hand  or  a  Fourth  on  the  other.  My  own 
experiments  with  stopped  organ  pipes  justify  me  in  asserting  that  however  much 
this  conclusion  is  opposed  to  musical  dogmas,  it  is  borne  out  by  the  fact,  provided 
that  really  simple  tones  are  used  for  the  purpose.ff  It  is  the  same  with  intervals 
near  to  the  major  Sixth ;  these  also  shew  no  difference  as  long  as  they  remain 
sufficently  far  from  the  Fifth  and  Octave.  Hence  although  it  is  not  difficult  to 
tune  perfect  major  and  minor  Thirds  on  the  harmonium  or  reed  pipes  or  on  the 
violin,  by  sounding  the  two  tones  together  and  trying  to  get  rid  of  the  beats,  it  is 
perfectly  impossible  to  do  so  on  stopped  organ  pipes  or  tuning-forks  without  the 

%  aid  of  other  intervals.    It  will  appear  hereafter  that  the  use  of  more  than  two 
tones  will  allow  these  interval3  to  be  perfectly  tuned  even  for  simple  tones. 

Intermediate  between  the  compound  tones  possessing  many  powerful  upper 
partials,  such  as  those  of  reed  pipes  and  violins,  and  the  entirely  simple  tones  of 
tuning-forks  and  stopped  organ  pipes,  lie  those  compound  tones  in  which  only  the 

*  [Bat,  as  before,  the  pitch  is  veiy  difife*  much  lower  in  pitch  and  so  inharmonic  to  the 

rent. — Translator,]  others  that  there  is  no  danger  of  oonfusing 

t  [Scheibler,  ibid.  p.  21.    I  myself  sac-  them.— Trafwtotor.] 
oeeded  in  hearing  and  counting  them.— 2Van«-  **  [Scheibler,  ibid,  p.  25,  says  only  *  as  beats 

lator,']  of  this  kind  are  too  indistinct/  he  uses  another 

X  [Scheibler  says,  p.  24,  they  are  heard  as  method  for  tuning   the    major  Third.     See 

well  as  for  the  Fifth.    I  have  not  found  it  so.  note  *,  p.  203^2.    He  also  calculates  the  int^r- 

^-Translator,']  mediate  tones  differently.    But  neither  he  nor 

§   [Supposing  the  pitch  numbers  of  the  any  one  seems  to  have  tried  to  verify  their 

mistuned  Fourth  are  300  and  401,  then  the  existence,  which  is  doubtful. — Translator,^ 
beating  upper    partials  would  be    1200  and  ff  [Or  at  any  rate  tones  without  the  4th 

1203,  a  very   high  pitch;   but  the  beating  partial,  which  those  of  stopped  organ  pipes  do 

differentials  are  202  and  199,  which  are  so  not  possess. — Translator.] 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


CHAP.. XI,  BEATS  DUE  TO  COMBINATIONAL  TONES.  aoi 

lowest  of  the  upper  partials  are  audible,  such  as  the  tones  of  wide  open  organ  pipes 
or  the  human  voice  when  singing  some  of  the  obscurer  vowels,  as  oo  in  too.  For 
these  the  partials  would  not  suffice  to  distinguish  all  the  consonant  intervals,  but 
the  addition  of  the  first  differential  tones  renders  it  possible. 

A.  Compound  Tones  consisting  of  the  prime  and  its  Octave,  These  cannot 
delimit  Fifths  and  Fourths  by  beats  of  the  partials,  but  are  able  to  do  so  by  those 
of  the  first  differential  tones. 

a.  Fifth.  Let  the  vibrational  numbers  of  the  prime  tones  be  200  and  301, 
which  are  accompanied  by  their  Octaves  400  and  602  ;  aU  four  tones  are  then  too 
for  apart  to  beat.    But  the  differential  tones 

301  —  200=101 
400—301=  99 

Difference    2  ^ 

give  two  beats.  The  number  of  these  beats  again  is  precisely  the  same  as  if  they 
had  been  produced  by  the  two  next  upper  partials.*    Namely 

2  X  301—3x200=2 

b.  Fourth,  Let  the  vibrational  numbers  of  the  primes  be  300  and  401,  and  of 
the  first  upper  partials  600  and  802  ;  these  cannot  produce  any  beats.  But  the 
first  differential  tones  give  3  beats,  thus  f : — 

600—401  =  199 
802—600=202 

Difference    3 

For  Thirds  it  would  be  necessary  to  take  differential  tones  of  the  second  ordei* 
into  account. 

B.  Compound  Tones  consisting  of  the  prime  and  Twelfth.    Such  tones  are 
produced  by  the  narrow  stopped  pipes  on  the  organ  {Quintaten,  p.  33^^,  note).   These  ^ 
are  related  in  the  same  way  as  those  which  have  only  the  Octave. 

a.  Fifth,  Primes  200  and  301,  upper  partials  600  and  903.    First  differential 

tone  / 

903—600=303 

Fifth=3oi 
Number  of  beats    2 

b.  Fourth,  Primes  300  and  401,  upper  partials  900  and  1203.  First  dif- 
ferential tone  * 

1203-900=303 
Lower  prime=3oo 

Number  of  beats    3 

Even  in  this  case  the  beats  of  the  Third  cannot  be  perceived  without  the  help 
of  the  weak  second  differential  tones.  11 

C.  Compound  Tones  having  both  Octave  and  Twelfth  as  audible  partials.  Such 
tones  are  produced  by  the  wide  (wooden)  open  pipes  of  the  organ  (Principal^ 
p,  93^^',  note).  The  beats  of  the  upper  partials  here  suffice  to  delimit  the  Fifths, 
bat  not  the  Fourths.  The  Thirds  can  now  be  distinguished  by  means  of  the  first 
differential  tones. 

a.  Major  Third.  Primes  400  and  501,  with  the  Octaves  800  and  1002,  and 
Twelfths  1200  and  1503.    First  differential  tones  J 

1002—800^202 
1200—1002^198 

Number  of  beats    4 

*  [The  same  in  number,  but  observe  that  existed  would  beat  at  pitch  1200,— Translator,] 
the  first  set  of  beats  are  at  pitch  100,  and  the  t  [These  are  the  same  two  beating  tones 

second  at  pitch  600.— Translator.]  as  calculated  on  p.  2006,  but  they  are  quite  dif- 

t  [At  pitch  200,  whereas  the  partials  if  they  ferently  deiiyed,—TranslatorJ] 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


202  BEATS  DUE  TO  COMBINATIONAL  TONES.  part  n. 

b.  Minor  Third.  Primes  500  and  601,  Octaves  1202,  Twelfths  1500  and  1803. 
Differential  tones  * 

1500—1202=298 
1803  —  1500=303 

Number  of  beats    5 

c.  Major  Sixth,  Primes  300  and  501,  Octaves  600  and  1002,  Twelfths  900  and 
1503.    Differential  tones 

600—501=  99 
1002  —  900=102 

Number  of  beats    3 

In  fact  not  only  the  beats  of  imperfect  Fifths  and  Fourths,  but  abo  those  of 
^  imperfect  major  and  minor  Thirds  are  easily  heard  on  open  organ  pipes,  and  can  be 
immediately  used  for  the  purposes  of  tuning. 

Thus,  where  upper  partials,  owing  to  the  quality  of  tone,  do  not  suffice,  the 
combinational  tones  step  in  to  make  every  imperfection  in  the  consonant  intervals 
of  the  Octave,  Fifth,  Fourth,  major  Sixth,  major  and  minor  Third  immediately 
sensible  by  means  of  beats  and  roughness  in  the  combined  sound,  and  thus  to  dis« 
tinguish  these  intervals  from  all  those  adjacent  to  them.  It  is  only  perfectly  simple 
tones  that  so  fsur  make  default  in  determining  the  Thirds  ;  and  for  them  also  the 
beats  which  disturb  the  harmoniousness  of  imperfect  Fifths  and  Fourths,  are 
relatively  too  weak  to  affect  the  ear  sensibly,  because  they  depend  on  differential 
tones  of  higher  orders.  In  reality,  as  I  have  already  mentioned,  two  stopped  pipes, 
giving  tones  which  lie  between  a  major  and  a  minor  Third  apart,  will  give  just  as 
good  a  consonance  as  if  the  interval  were  exactly  either  a  major  or  a  minor  Third. 
This  does  not  mean  that  a  practised  musical  ear  would  not  find  such  an  interval 
V  strange  and  unusual,  and  hence  would  perhaps  call  it  false,  but  the  immediate  im« 
pression  on  the  ear,  the  simple  perception  of  harmoniousness,  considered  indepen- 
dently of  any  musical  habits,  is  in  no  respect  worse  than  for  one  of  the  perfect 
intervals.! 

Matters  are  very  different  when  more  than  two  simple  tones  are  sounded 
together.  We  have  seen  that  Octaves  are  precisely  limited  even  for  simple  tones 
by  the  beats  of  the  first  differential  tone  with  the  lower  primary.  Now  suppose 
that  an  Octave  has  been  tuned  perfectly,  and  that  then  a  third  tone  is  interposed 
to  act  as  a  FiftL  Then  if  the  Fifth  is  not  perfect,  beats  will  ensue  from  the  first 
differential  tone. 

Let  the  tones  forming  the  perfect  Octave  have  the  pitch  numbers  200  and  400, 
and  let  that  of  the  imperfect  Fifth  be  301.    The  differential  tones  are 

400-301=  99 

301  —  200=101 

H  

Number  of  beats    2 

These  beats  of  the  Fifth  which  lies  between  two  Octaves  are  much  more 
audible  than  those  of  the  Fifth  alone  without  its  Octave.  The  latter  depend  on 
the  weak  differential  tones  of  the  second  order,  the  former  on  those  of  the  first 
order.  Hence  Scheibler  some  time  ago  laid  down  the  rule  for  tuning  tuning-forks, 
first  to  tune  two  of  them  as  a  perfect  Octave,  and  then  to  sound  them  both  at 
once  with  the  Fifth,  in  order  to  tune  the  latter .4:  If  Fifth  and  Octave  are  both 
perfect,  they  also  give  together  the  perfect  Fourth. 

The  case  is  similar,  when  two  simple  tones  have  been  tuned  to  be  a  perfect 

*  [This  was  not  given  for  simple  tones  be-  for  oases  where  neither  partial  nor  combina- 

fore,  but  Scheibler  calculates  the  result  in  that  tional  tones  are  present,  App.  XX.  sect.  L. 

case,  p.  26,  and  says  he  could  use  it  still  less  art.  7. — Translator,] 
than  for  the  major  Third.—Translator.]  %  (l  have  been  unable  to  find  the  passage 

t  [See  Prof.  Preyer's  theory  of  consonance  referred  to.-  Translator.' 

Digitized  by  V^OOQIC 


CHAP. «.  BEATS  DUE  TO  COMBINATIONAL  TONES.  203 

Fifth,  and  we  interpoee  a  new  tone  between  them  to  act  as  a  major  Third.  Let  the 
perfect  Fifth  have  the  pitch  numbers  400  and  600.  On  intercalating  the  impure 
major  Third  with  the  pitch  number  501  in  lieu  of  500,  the  differential  tones  are 

600—501=  99 
501—400=101 

Number  of  beats    2* 

The  major  Sixth  is  determined  by  combining  it  with  the  Fourth.  Let  300  and 
^00  be  the  vibrational  numbers  of  a  perfect  Fourth,  and  501  that  of  an  imperfect 
major  Sixth.    The  differential  tones  are 

501—400=101 
400—300=100 

—  H 

Number  of  beats    i 

If  we  tried  to  intercalate  an  interval  between  the  tones  forming  a  perfect 
Fourth,  and  having  the  vibrational  numbers  300  and  400,  it  could  onlyl)e  the  sub- 
minor  Third  with  the  vibrational  number  350.  Taking  it  imperfect  and  =351,  we 
have  the  differential  tones 

400-351=49 

351-300=51 

Number  of  beats  2 

These  intervals  8  :  7  and  7  :  6  are,  however,  too  close  to  be  consonances,  and 
hence  they  can  only  be  used  in  weak  discords  (chord  of  the  dominant  Seventh),  f 

The  above  considerations  are  also  applicable  to  any  single  compound  tone  con- 
sisting of  several  partials.  Any  two  partials  of  sufficient  force  will  also  produce  ^ 
differential  tones  in  the  ear.  If,  then,  the  partials  correspond  exactly  to  the  series 
of  harmonic  partials,  as  assigned  by  the  series  of  smaUer  whole  numbers,  all  these 
differentials  resulting  &om  partials  coincide  exactly  with  the  partials  themselves, 
and  give  no  beats.  Thus  if  the  prime  makes  n  vibrations  ^in  a  second,  the  upper 
partials  make  2n,  371,  4n,  &c.,  vibrations,  and  the  differences  of  these  numbers  are 
again  n,  or  271,  or  371,  &c.  The  pitch  numbers  of  the  summational  tones  fall  also 
into  this  series. 

On  the  other  hand,  if  the  pitch  numbers  of  the  upper  partials  are  ever  so 
slightly  different  from,  those  giving  these  ratios,  then  the  combinational  tones  will 
differ  from  one  another  and  from  the  upper  partials,  and  the  result  will  be  beats. 
The  tone  therefore  ceases  to  make  that  uniform  and  quiet  impression  which  a 
compound  tone  with  harmonic  upper  partials  always  makes  on  the  ear.  How  con- 
siderable this  influence  is,  we  may  hear  from  any  firmly  attached  harmonious 
string  after  we  have  fastened  a  small  piece  of  wax  on  any  part  of  its  length.  This^  f 
as  theory  and  experiment  alike  shew,  produces  an  inharmonic  relation  of  the 
upper  partials.  If  the  piece  of  wax  is  very  small,  then  the  alteration  of  tone  is 
also  very  small.  But  the  slightest  mistuning  suffices  to  do  considerable  harm 
to  the  tunefulness  of  the  sound,  and  renders  the  tone  dull  and  rough,  like  a  tin 
kettle. 

*  [On  this  was  fonnded  8cheLbler*8  method  asing  the  perfect  Fifth,  A  220,  Ct  277-1824, 
of  taniiig  the  perfect  major  Third  (alluded  to  -£^330.  Then,  277'i824-220« 57-1824,  330— 
in  p.  20od\  note)  and  also  the  tempered  major  277*1824  =  52*81 76  and  57*  1824  »  52-81 76  =: 
G?hird.  4*3648,  and  hence  the  tuning  of  tiie  inter- 
First  tone  a  perfect  Fifth,  and  then  an  mediate  fork  must  be  altered  till  these  beats 
auxiliary  Fifth,  2  vib.  sharper.  Then  if  the  are  heard.  These  are  Scheibler's  own  ex- 
major  Third  is  perfect  we  have  A  220,  CZ  275,  amples,  p.  26,  reduced  to  ordinary  double 
E  332  and  275-220-55,  332-275*57,  and  vibrations.— Tnan^Za^.] 
^7—55  =  2.  Hence  the  tuning  of  C%  must  f  [In  actual  practice,  for  the  chord  of  the 
be  altered  till  the  differential  tones  beat  2  in  dominant  Seventh  the  interval  is  4  :  7|  the  in- 
a  second.  terval  of  the  just  subminor  Seventh  4  :  7  not  be- 
For  the  tempered  major  Third  we  have,  ing  used,  even  in  just  intonation.— jTrafuZa^or.j 


Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


i 


ao4  BEATS  DUE  TQ  COMBINATIONAL  TONEa  »bt  n. 

Herein  we  find  the  reason  why  tones  with  harmonic  upper  partials  play  such  a 
leading  part  in  the  sensation  of  the  ear.  They  are  the  only  sounds  which,  even 
when  very  intense,  can  produce  sensations  that  continue  in  undisturhed  repose, 
without  beats,  corresponding  to  the  purely  periodic  motion  of  the  air,  which  is  the 
objective  foundation  of  these  tones.  I  have  already  stated  as  a  result  of  the 
summary  which  I  gave  of  the  composition  of  musical  tones  in  Chapter  V.,  No.  2, 
p.  1 19a,  that  besides  tones  with  harmonic  upper  partials,  the  only  others  used  (and 
that  also  generally  in  a  very  subordinate  manner)  are  either  such  as  have  a  section 
of  the  series  of  harmonic  upper  partials,  (like  those  of  well  tuned  bells),  or  such 
as  have  secondary  tones  (as  those  in  bars)  so  very  weak  and  so  fax  distant  from 
their  primes,  that  their  differentials  have  but  little  force  and  at  any  rate  do  not 
produce  any  distinct  beats. 

Collecting  the  results  of  our  investigations  upon  beats,  we  find  that  when  two 
^  or  more  simple  tones  are  sounded  at  the  same  time,  they  cannot  go  on  sounding 
without  mutual  disturbance,  unless  they  form  with  each  other  certain  perfectly 
definite  intervals.  Such  an  undisturbed  flow  of  simultaneous  tones  is  called  a 
consonance.  When  these  intervals  do  not  exist,  beats  arise,  that  is,  the  whole 
compound  tones,  or  individual  partial  and  combinational  tones  contained  in  them 
or  resulting  from  them,  alternately  reinforce  and  enfeeble  each  other.  The  tones 
then  do  not  coexist  undisturbed  in  the  ear.  They  mutually  check  each  other's 
uniform  flow.    This  process  is  called  dissonance,* 

Combinational  ^/^rma  t^n^  fti^  lyn^af  por^nffri  nonc^f,  pf  t^^atfi,  They  are  the  sole 
cause  of  beats  for  simple  tones  which  lie  as  much  as,  or  more  than,  a  minor  Third 
apart.f  For  two  simple  tones  they  suffice  to  delimit  the  Fifth,  perhaps  the 
Fourth,  but  certainly  not  the  Thirds  and  Sixths.  These,  however,  will  be  strictly 
delimited  when  the  major  Third  is  added  to  the  Fifth  to  form  the  common  major 
chord,  and  when  the  Sixth  is  united  with  the  Fourth  to  form  the  chord  of  the 

1  Sixth  and  Fourth,  ^. 
4 

Thirds,  however,  are  strictly  delimited,  by  means  of  the  beats  of  imperfect 
intervals,  in  a  chord  of  two  compound  tones,  each  consisting  of  a  prime  and  the 
two  next  partial  tones.  The  beats  of  such  intervals  increase  in  strength  and  dis- 
tinctness, with  the  increase  in  number  and  strength  of  the  upper  partial  tones 
in  the  compounds.  By  this  means  the  difference  between  dissonance  and  conso- 
nance, and  of  perfectly  from  imperfectly  tuned  intervals,  becomes  continually  more 
marked  and  distinct,  increasing  the  certainty  with  which  the  hearer  distinguishes 
the  correct  intervals,  and  adding  much  to  the  powerful  and  artistic  effect  of  succes- 
sions of  chords.  Finally  when  the  high  upper  partials  are  relatively  too  strong  (in 
piercing  and  braying  qualities  of  tone)  each  separate  tone  will  by  itself  generate 
intermittent  sensations  of  tone,  and  any  combination  of  two  or  more  compounds  of 
this  description  produces  a  sensible  increase  of  this  harshness,  while  at  the  same 
f  time  the  large  number  of  partial  and  combinational  tones  renders  it  difficult  for  the 
hearer  to  follow  a  complicated  arrangement  of  parts  in  a  musical  composition. 

These  relations  are  of  the  utmost  importance  for  the  use  of  different  instru- 
ments in  the  different  kinds  of  musical  composition.  The  considerations  which 
determine  the  selection  of  the  proper  instrument  for  an  entire  composition  or  for 
individual  phrases  in  movements  written  for  an  orchestra  are  very  multi&rious. 
First  in  rank  stands  mobility  and  power  of  tone  in  the  different  instruments.  On 
this  there  is  no  need  to  dwell.  The  bowed  instruments  and  pianoforte  surpass  all 
others  in  mobility,  and  then  follow  the  flutes  and  oboes.  To  these  are  opposed  the 
trumpets  and  trombones,  which  conunence  sluggishly,  but  surpass  all  instruments 
in  power.  Another  essential  consideration  is  expressiveness,  which  in  general 
depends  on  the  power  of  producing  with  certainty  any  degree  of  rapid  alterations 
in  loudness  at  the  pleasure  of  the  player.    In  this  respect  also  bowed  instruments, 

♦  [See  Prof.  Preyer'g  addendum  to  this  f  [But  see  App.  XX.  sect.  L.  art.  3.— Traiw- 

theoryinApp.XX.sect.Ij.  art.  7.  — rrawiotor.]      lator.] 

Digitized  by  V^OOQIC 


.fjHAP.xi/ DISSONANCE  FOR  DIFFERENT  QUALITIES  OF  TONE.   20J 

and  the  human  voice,  are  pre-eminent.  Artificial  reed  instruments,  both  of  wood 
and  brass,  cannot  materiallj  diminish  their  power  without  stopping  the  action  of 
the  reeds.  Flutes  and  organ  pipes  cannot  greatly  alter  the  force  of  their  tone 
without  at  the  same  time  altering  their  pitch.  On  the  pianoforte  the  strength  with 
which  a  tone  commences  is  determined  by.  the  player,  but  not  its  duration ;  so  that 
the  rhythm  can  be  marked  delicately,  but  real  melodic  expression  is  wanting.  All 
these  points  in  the  use  of  the  above  instruments  are  easy  to  observe  and  have  long 
been  known  and  allowed  for.  The  influence  of  quality  proper  was  more  difficult 
to  define.  Our  investigations,  however,  on  the  composition  of  musical  tones  have 
given  us  a  means  of  taking  into  account  the  principal  differences  in  the  effect  of 
the  simultaneous  action  of  different  instruments  and  of  shewing  how  the  problem 
is  to  be  solved,  although  there  is  still  a  large  field  left  for  a  searching  investigation 
in  detail. 

Let  us  begin  with  the  simple  tones  of  v)ide  stopped  organ  pipes.  In  themselves  ^ 
they  are  very  soft  and  mild,  dull  in  the  low  notes,  and  very  tuneful  in  the  upper. 
They  are  quite  unsuited,  however,  for  combinations  of  harmony  according  to 
modem  musical  theory.  We  have  already  explained  that  simple  tones  of  this  kind 
discriminate  only  the  very  small  interval  of  a  Second  by  strong  beats.  Imperfect 
Octaves,  and  the  dissonant  intervals  in  the  neighbourhood  of  the  Octave,  (the  'p 
Sevenths  and  Ninths,)  beat  with  the  combinational  tones,  but  these  beats  are 
weak  in  comparison  with  those  due  to  upper  partials.  The  beats  of  imperfect 
Fifths  and  Fourths  are  entirely  inaudible  except  under  the  most  favourable  condi- 
tions. Hence  in  general  the  impression  made  on  the  ear  by  any  dissonant  interval, 
except  the  Second,  differs  very  little  from  that  made  by  consonances,  and  as  a 
consequence  the  harmony  loses  its  character  and  the  hearer  has  no  certainty  in  his 
perception  of  the  difference  of  intervals.*  If  polyphonic  compositions  containing 
the  harshest  and  most  venturesome  dissonances  are  played  upon  wide  stopped 
organ  pipes,  the  whole  is  uniformly  soft  and  harmonious,  and  for  that  very  reason  ^ 
also  indefinite,  wearisome  and  weak,  without  character  or  energy.  Every  reader 
that  has  an  opporttmity  is  requested  to  try  this  experiment.  There  is  no  better 
proof  of  the  important  part  which  upper  partial  tones  play  in  music,  than  the  im- 
pression produced  by  music  composed  of  simple  tones,  such  as  we  have  just 
described.  Hence  the  wide  stopped  pipes  of  the  organ  are  used  only  to  give 
prominence  to  the  extreme  softness  and  tunefcdness  of  certain  phrases  in  contra- 
distinction to  the  harsher  effect  of  other  stops,  or  else,  in  connection  with  other 
stops,  to  strengthen  their  prime  tones.  Next  to  the  wide  stopped  organ  pipes  as 
regards  quality  of  tone  stand  flutes  and  the  flxie  pipes  on  organs  (open  pipes,  blown 
gently).  These  have  the  Octave  plainly  in  addition  to  the  prime,  and  when  blown 
more  strongly  even  produce  the  Twelfth.  In  this  case  the  Octaves  and  Fifths  are 
more  distinctly  delimited  by  upper  partial  tones  ;  but  the  definition  of  Thirds  and 
Sixths  has  to  depend  upon  combinational  tones,  and  hence  is  much  weaker.  The 
musical  character  of  these  pipes  is  therefore  not  much  unlike  that  of  the  wide  ^ 
stopped  pipes  already  described.  This  is  well  expressed  by  the  old  joke  that  nothing 
is  more  dreadful  to  a  musical  ear  than  a  flute-concerto,  except  a  concerto  for  two 
flutes.f  But  in  combination  with  other  instruments  which  give  effect  to  the  con- 
nection of  the  harmony,  the  flute,  from  the  perfect  softness  of  its  tone  and  its 
great  mobility,  is  extraordinarily  pleasant  and  attractive,  and  cannot  be  replaced 
by  any  other  instrument.  In  ancient  music  the  flute  played  a  much  more  im- 
portant part  than  at  present,  and  this  seems  to  accord  with  the  whole  ideal  of 
classical  art,  which  aimed  at  keeping  every  thing  unpleasant  from  its  productions, 
eonfining  itself  to  pure  beauty,  whereas  modem  art  requires  more  abundant  means 

*  [Bnt  see  Prof.  Preyer  in  App.  XX.  sect.  L.  a  concerto  or  peculiar  piece  of  music  for  one 

art.  7. — Travslator,]  instrument,  and  secondly  as  a  concert^  or  piece 

f  [In  the  original,  *  dass  einem  musikali-  of  music  for  several  instruments,  cannot  be 

0chen  Ohre  nichts  sohrecklicher  sei  als  ein  properly  rendered  in  the  translation. — Tram- 

Fldtenconcert,  ausgenommen  ein  Ck)ncert  von  lator,'\ 
z-wei  Floten.'    The  pun  on  '  Concert,'  first  as 


Digitized  by 


Google 


2o6     DISSONANCE  FOR  DIFFERENT  QUALITIES  OF  TONE,  pabt  n. 

of  expression,  and  consequently  to  a  certain  extent  admits  into  its  circle  what  in 
itself  would  be  contrary  to  the  gratification  of  the  senses.  However  this  be,  the 
earnest  friends  of  music,  even  in  classical  times,  contended  for  the  harsher  tones 
of  stringed  instruments  in  opposition  to  the  effeminate  flute. 

The  open  organ  pipes  afford  a  favourable  means  of  meeting  the  harmonic 
requirements  of  polyphonic  music,  and  consequently  form  the  principal  stops.* 
They  make  the  lower  partials  distinctly  audible,  the  wide  pipes  up  to  the  third,  the 
narrow  ones  (geigen  principal  t)  up  to  the  sixth  partial  tone.  The  wider  pipes 
have  more  power  of  tone  than  the  narrower ;  to  give  them  more  brightness  the 
8-foot  stops,  which  contain  the  '  principal  work,'  are  connected  with  the  4-foot 
stops,  which  add  the  Octave  to  each  note,  or  the  principal  is  connected  with  the 
geigen  principal,  so  that  the  first  gives  power  and  the  second  brightness.  By  this 
means  qualities  of  tone  are  produced  which  contain  the  first  six  partial  tones  in 
%  moderate  force,  decreasing  as  the  pitch  ascends.  These  give  a  very  distinct  feeling 
for  the  purity  of  the  consonant  intervals,  enabling  us  to  distinguish  clearly  between 
consonance  and  dissonance,  and  preventing  the  unavoidable  but  weak  dissonances 
that  result  from  the  higher  upper  partials  in  the  imperfect  consonances,  from  be- 
coming too  marked,  but  at  the  same  time  not  allowing  the  hearer's  appreciation  of 
the  progression  of  the  parts  to  be  disturbed  by  a  multitude  of  loud  accessory  tones. 
In  this  respect  the  organ  has  an  advantage  over  all  other  instruments,  as  the 
player  is  able  to  mix  and  alter  the  quahties  of  tone  at  pleasure,  and  make  them 
suitable  to  the  character  of  the  piece  he  has  to  perform. 

The  narrow  stopped  pipes  (Quintaten),t  for  which  the  prime  tone  is  ac- 
companied by  the  Twelfth,  the  reed-flute  (Bohrfiote)  §  where  the  third  and  fifth 
partial  are  both  present,  the  conical  open  pipes,  as  the  goat-horn  (Gemshom),** 
which  reinforce  certain  higher  partials  ft  more  than  the  lower,  and  so  forth,  serve 
only  to  give  distinctive  qualities  of  tone  for  particular  parts,  and  thus  to  separate 
f  them  from  the  rest.  They  are  not  weU  adapted  for  forming  the  chief  mass  of  the 
harmony. 

Very  piercing  quahties  of  tone  are  produced  by  the  reed  pipes  and  compound 
stops  tt  on  the  organ.  The  latter,  as  already  explained,  are  artificial  imitations  of 
the  natural  composition  of  all  musical  tones,  each  key  bringing  a  series  of  pipes 
into  action,  which  correspond  to  the  first  three  or  first  six  partial  tones  of  the 
corresponding  note.  They  can  be  used  only  to  accompany  congregational  singing. 
When  employed  alone  they  produce  insupportable  noise  and  horrible  confusion. 
But  when  the  singing  of  the  congregation  gives  overpowering  force  to  the  prime 
tones  in  the  notes  of  the  melody,  the  proper  relation  of  quahty  of  tone  is  restored, 
and  the  result  is  a  powerful,  well-proportioned  mass  of  sound.  Without  the 
assistance  of  these  compound  stops  it  would  be  impossible  to  control  a  vast  body 
of  sound  produced  by  unpractised  voices,  such  as  we  hear  in  [German]  churches. 

The  human  voice  is  on  the  whole  not  unhke  the  organ  in  quahty,  so  for  as 
^  harmony  is  concerned.  The  brighter  vowels,  of  course,  generate  isolated  high 
partial  tones,  but  these  are  so  unconnected  with  the  rest  that  they  can  have  no 
universal  and  essential  effect  on  the  sound  of  the  chords.  For  this  we  must  look 
to  the  lower  partials,  which  are  tolerably  uniform  for  all  vowels.  But  of  course  in 
particular  consonances  the  characteristic  tone  of  the  vowels  may  play  an  important 
part.  If,  for  example,  two  human  voices  sing  the  major  Third  ^  <)'  on  the  vowel 
a  in  father,  the  fourth  partial  of  hj;}  (or  b']}),  and  the  third  partial  of  d'  (or  a*%  £eJ1 
among  the  tones  characteristically  reinforced  by  A,  and  consequently  the  imperfec- 
tion of  the  consonance  of  a  major  Third  will  come  out  harshly  by  the  dissonance 
a"  b']},  between  these  upper  partials  ;  whereas  if  the  vowel  be  changed  to  0  in  no, 
the  dissonance  disappears.    On  the  other  hand  the  Fourth  ^  e'[>  soimds  perfectly 

♦  [See  p.  141^',  note  ^.—Translator.]  *♦  [See  p.  94^,  note.— Translator.] 

t  [See  p.  141(2,  note.— TranaZator.]  ff  [Generally    the  4th,    6th,  and    7th.— 

t  [See  p.  33d,  note.— Translat/>r.]  Translator.] 

§  [See  p.  94<?',  note.— TransZafor.]  tt  [See  p»  57a',  note.-— Tniwsiator.] 

Digitized  by  V^OOQIC 


OHAP.  XI.   DISSONANCE   FOR  DIFFERENT  QUALITIES  OF  TONE.    207 

weU  on  the  vowel  a  in  father,  because  the  higher  note  e'\}  has  the  same  upper  par- 
tial b"\}  as  the  deeper  b\}.  But  if  a  in  father  being  incHned  towards  a  in  fally  or  a  in 
fat,  the  upper  partials  /'  and  e"\}  or  else  d'"  and  e'"\}  might  interrupt  the  con- 
sonance. This  serves  to  shew,  among  other  things,  that  the  translation  of  the 
words  of  a  song  from  one  language  into  another  is  not  by  any  means  a  matter 
of  indifference  for  its  musical  effect.* 

Disregarding  at  present  these  reinforcements  of  partials  due  to  the  characteristic 
resonance  of  each  vowel,  the  musical  tones  of  the  human  voice  are  on  the  whole 
accompanied  by  the  lower  partials  in  moderate  strength,  and  hence  are  well  adapted 
for  combinations  of  chords,  precisely  as  the  tones  of  the  principal  stops  of  the 
organ.  Besides  this  the  human  voice  has  a  peculiar  advantage  over  the  organ  and 
all  other  musical  instruments  in  the  execution  of  polyphonic  music.  The  words 
which  are  sung  connect  the  notes  belonging  to  each  part,  and  form  a  clue  which 
readily  guides  the  hearer  to  discover  and  pursue  the  related  parts  of  the  whole  body  ^ 
of  sound.  Hence  polyphonic  music  and  the  whole  modem  system  of  harmony 
were  first  developed  on  the  human  voice.  Indeed,  nothing  can  exceed  the  musical 
effect  of  well  harmonised  part  music  perfectly  executed  in  just  intonation  by  prac- 
tised voices.  For  the  complete  harmoniousness  of  such  music  it  is  indispensably 
necessary  that  the  several  musical  intervals  should  be  justly  intoned,  and  our  pre- 
sent singersf  unfortunately  seldom  learn  to  take  just  intervals,  because  they  are 
accustomed  from  the  first  to  sing  to  the  accompaniment  of  instruments  which  are 
tuned  in  equal  temperament,  and  hence  with  imperfect  consonances.  It  is  only 
such  singers  as  have  a  delicate  musical  feeling  of  their  own  who  find  out  the 
correct  result,  which  is  no  longer  taught  them. 

Richer  in  upper  partials,  and  consequently  brighter  in  tone  than  the  human 
voice  and  the  principal  stops  on  the  organ,  are  the  bowed  instruments,  which  con- 
sequently fill  such  an  important  place  in  music.  Their  extraordinary  mobihty  and 
expressiveness  give  them  the  first  place  in  instrumental  music,  and  the  moderate  ^ 
acuteness  of  their  quality  of  tone  assigns  them  an  intermediate  position  between 
the  softer  flutes  and  the  braying  brass  instruments.  There  is  a  slight  difference 
between  the  different  instruments  of  this  class ;  the  tenor  and  double-bass  have  a 
somewhat  acuter  and  thinner  quality  than  the  violin  and  violoncello,  that  is,  they 
have  relatively  stronger  upper  partials.  The  audible  partials  reach  to  the  sixth  or 
eighth,  according  as  the  bow  is  brought  nearer  the  finger-board  for  piano,  or  nearer 
the  bridge  for  forte,  and  they  decrease  regularly  in  force  as  they  ascend  in  pitch. 
Hence  on  bowed  instruments  the  difference  between  consonance  and  dissonance  is 

*  [Also,  it  shews  how  the  musical  effect  of  and  tempered  intonation  in  the  singing  of  the 

difTerent  stanzas  in  a  ballad,  though  sung  to  the  same  choir.     It  was  a  choir  of  about  60  mixed 

same  written  notes,  will  constantly  vary,  quite  voices,  which  had  gained  the  prize  at  the  In- 

independently  of  difference  of  expression.  This  ternational  Exhibition  at  Paris  in  1867,  and 

is  often  remarkable  on  the  closing  cadence  of  had  been  kept  well  together  ever  since.    After 

the  stanza.    As  the  vowel  changes  from  a  in  singing  some  pieces  without  accompaniment.  ^ 

father,  to  a  in  mat ;  e  in  viet,  or  i  in  sit,  or  and  hence  in  the  just  intonation  to  which  the 

again  to  o  in  not,  u  in  hut,  and  u  in  put,  the  singers  had  been  trained,  and  with  the  most 

musical  result  is  totally  different,  thou,r^h  the  delightful  effect  of  harmony,  they  sang  a  piece 

pitch  remains  unaltered.    To  shew  the  effect  with  a  pianoforte  accompaniment.    Of  course 

of  the  different  vowels  throughout  a  piece  of  the  pianoforte  itself  was  inaudible  among  the 

music,  I  asked  a  set  of  about  8  voices  to  sing,  mass  of  sound  produced  by  sixty  voices.    But 

before  about  200  others,  the  first  half  of  See  it  had  the  effect  of  perverting  their  intonation, 

the  conquering  hero  comes,  first  to  lah,  then  to  and  the  whole  charm  of  the  singing  was  at 

lee,  and  then  to  loo.     The  difference  of  effect  once  destroyed.     There  was  nothing  left  but 

was  almost  ludicrous.    Much  has  to  be  studied  the  everyday  singing   of   an   ordinary  choir, 

in  the  relation  of  the  qualities  of  vowels  to  the  The  disillusion  was  complete  and  the  effect 

effect  of  the  music.     In  this  respect,  too,  the  most  unsatisfactory  as  a   conclusion.     If  the 

pitch  chosen  for  the  tonic  will  be  found  of  great  same  piece  of  music  or  succession  of  chords  in 

importance. — Translator.]  C  major  or  C  minor,  without  any  modulation, 

t  I^This  refers  to  Germany,  not  to  the  Eng-  be  played  first  on  the  Harmonical  and  then  be 

lish  Tonic  Soltaists,  nor  to  the  English  ma-  contrasted  with   an   ordinary   tempered  har- 

drigal  singers.    On  Dec.  27,  1869,  at  a  meeting  monium,  the  same  kind  of  difference  will  be 

of  the  Tonic  Solfa  College  I  had  an  unusual  felt,  but  not  so  strongly.  —  Tran^Aitor.J 
opportunity  of  contrasting  the  effect  of   just 


Digitized  by 


Google 


2o8    DISSONANCE   FOR  DIFFERENT  QUALITIES   OF  TONE,    part  ii. 

clearly  and  distinctly  marked,  and  the  feeling  for  the  justness  of  the  intervals  very 
certain  ;  indeed  it  is  notorious  that  practised  viohn  and  violoncello  players  have  a 
very  delicate  ear  for  distinguishing  differences  of  pitch.     On  the  other  hand  the 
piercing  character  of  the  tones  is  so  marked,  that  soft  song-like  melodies  are  not 
well  suited  for  bowed  instruments,  and  are  better  given  to  flutes  and  clarinets  in 
the  orchestra.     Full  chords  are  also  relatively  too  rough,  since  those  upper  partials 
fs  which  form  dissonant  intervals  in  every  consonance,  are  sufficiently  strong  to  make 
\  the  dissonance  obtrusive,  especially  for  Thirds  and  Sixths.     Moreover,  the  im- 
\  perfect  Thirds  and  Sixths  of  the  tempered  musical  scale  are  on  bowed  instruments 
very  perceptibly  different  in  effect  from  the  justly  intoned  Thirds  and  Sixths  when 
.  the  player  does  not  know  how  to  substitute  the  pure  intervals  for  them,  as  the  ear 
requires.   Hence  in  compositions  for  bowed  instruments,  slow  and  flowing  progres- 
sions of  chords  are  introduced  by  way  of  exception  only,  because  they  are  not 
%  sufficiently  harmonious ;  quick  movements  and  figures,  and  arpeggio  chords  are 
preferred,  for  which  these  instruments  are  extremely  well  adapted,  and  in  which 
the  acute  and  piercing  character  of  their  combined  sounds  cannot  be  so  distinctly 
perceived. 

The  beats  have  a  pecuHar  character  in  the  case  of  bowed  instruments.  Regular, 
slow,  numerable  beats  seldom  occur.  This  is  owing  to  the  minute  irregularities  in 
the  action  of  the  bow  on  the  string,  abeady  described,  to  which  is  due  the  well- 
known  scraping  effect  so  often  heard.  Observations  on  the  vibrational  figure 
shew  that  every  httle  scrape  of  the  bow  causes  the  vibrational  curve  to  jump  sud- 
denly backwards  or  forwards,  or  in  physical  terms,  causes  a  sudden  alteration  in  the 
phase  of  vibration.  Now  since  it  depends  solely  on  the  difference  of  phase  whether 
two  tones  which  are  sounded  at  the  same  time  mutually  reinforce  or  enfeeble  each 
other,  every  minutest  catching  or  scraping  of  the  bow  will  also  affect  the  flow  of  the 
beats,  and  when  two  tones  of  the  same  pitch  are  played,  every  jump  in  the  phase  will 
^  suffice  to  produce  a  change  in  the  loudness,  just  as  if  irregular  beats  were  occurring 
at  unexpected  moments.  Hence  the  best  instruments  and  the  best  players  are 
necessary  to  produce  slow  beats  or  a  uniform  flow  of  sustained  consonant  chords. 
Probably  this  is  one  of  the  reasons  why  quartetts  for  bowed  instruments,  when 
executed  by  players  who  can  play  solo  pieces  pleasantly  enough,  sometimes  sound 
so  intolerably  rough  and  harsh  that  the  effect  bears  no  proper  ratio  to  the  slight 
roughness  wliich  each  individual  player  produces  on  his  own  instrument.*  When 
I  was  making  observations  on  vibrational  figures,  I  found  it  difficult  to  avoid  the 
occurrence  of  one  or  two  jumps  in  the  figure  every  second.  Now  in  solo-playing 
the  tone  of  the  string  is  thus  interrupted  for  almost  inappreciably  minute  instants, 
which  the  hearer  scarcely  perceives,  but  in  a  quartett  when  a  chord  is  played  for 
which  all  the  notes  have  a  common  upper  partial  tone,  there  would  be  from  four 
to  eight  sudden  and  irregular  alterations  of  loudness  in  this  common  tone  every 
second,  and  this  could  not  pass  unobserved.  Hence  for  good  combined  performance, 
f  a  much  greater  evenness  of  tone  is  required  than  for  solo-playing.f 

The  pianoforte  takes  the  first  place  among  stringed  instruments  for  which  the 
strings  are  struck.  The  previous  analysis  of  its  quahty  of  tone  shews  that  its 
deeper  octaves  are  rich,  but  its  higher  octaves  relatively  poor,  in  upper  partial  tones. 
In  the  lower  octaves,  the  second  or  third  partial  tone  is  often  as  loud  as  the  prime, 
nay,  the  second  partial  is  often  louder  than  the  prime.     The  consequence  is  that 

♦  [To  myself,  one  of  the  principal  reasons  rally  known.    If  the  music  notes  could  be 

for  the  painful  effect  here  alluded  to,  which  is  previously  marked  by  duodenals,  in  the  way 

unfortunately  so  extremely  well  known,  is  the  suggested  in  App.  XX.  sect.  E.  art.  26,  much 

fact  that  the  players  not  having  been  taught  the  of  this  difficulty  might  be  avoided  from  the 

nature  of  just  intonation,  do  not  accommodate  first.    But  the  marking  would  require  a  study 

the  pitches  of    the  notes  properly.      When  not  yet  commenced  by  the  greater  number  of 

quartett  players  are  used  to  one  another  they  rausici&ns.— Translator,] 
overcome  this  difficulty.    But  when  they  learn  f  [On    violins     combinational    tones    are 

thus,  it  is  a  mere  accommodation  of  the  different  strong.    I  have  been  told  that  violinists  watch 

intervals  by  ear  to  the  playing  of   (say)  the  for  the  Octave  differential  tone,  in  tuning  their 

leader.   (See  App.  XX.  sect.  G.  art.  7.)   The  real  Fitihs.—Trmislator.] 
relations  of  the  just  tones  are  in  fact  not  gene- 
Digitized  by  VjOOQIC 


CHAP.  XI.  DISSOKANCE  FOB  DIFFERENT  QUALITIES  OF  TONE.   209 

the  dissonances  near  the  Octave  (the  Sevenths  and  Ninths)  are  ahnost  as  harsh  as 
the  Seconds,  and  that  diminished  and  augmented  Twelfths  and  Fifths  are  rather 
rongh.  The  4th,  5th,  and  6th  partial  tones,  on  the  other  hand,  on  which  the  Thirds 
depend,  decrease  rapidly  in  force,  so  that  the  Thirds  are  relatively  much  less  dis  • 
tinctly  delimited  than  the  Octaves,  Fifths,  and  Fourths.  This  last  circumstance  is 
important,  because  it  makes  the  sharp  Thirds  of  the  equal  temperament  much  more 
endurable  upon  the  piano  than  upon  other  instruments  with  a  more  piercing  quahty 
of  tone,  whereas  the  Octaves,  Fifths,  and  Fourths  are  delimited  with  great  distinct- 
ness and  certainty.  Notwithstanding  the  relatively  large  number  of  upper  partial 
tones  on  the  pianoforte,  the  impression  produced  by  dissonances  is  fiar  from  being  so 
penetrating  as  on  instruments  of  long-sustained  tones.  On  the  piano  the  note  is 
powerful  only  at  the  moment  when  it  is  struck,  and  rapidly  decreases  in  strength, 
so  that  the  beats  which  characterise  the  dissonances  have  not  time  to  become 
sensible  during  the  strong  conmiencement  of  the  tone ;  they  do  not  even  begin  IT 
until  the  tone  is  greatly  diminished  in  intensity.  Hence  in  the  modem  music 
written  for  the  pianoforte,  since  the  time  that  Beethoven  shewed  how  the  cha- 
racteristic peculiarities  of  the  instrument  were  to  be  utilised  in  compositions,  we 
find  an  accumtdation  and  reduplication  of  dissonant  intervals  which  would  be  per- 
fectly insupportable  on  other  instruments.  The  great  difference  becomes  very  evi- 
dent when  an  attempt  is  made  to.  play  recent  compositions  for  the  piano  on  the 
harmonium  or  organ. 

That  instrument-makers,  led  solely  by  practised  ears,  and  not  by  any  theory, 
should  have  found  it  most  advantageous  to  arrange  the  striking  place  of  the 
hammer  so  that  the  7th  partial  tone  entirely  disappears,  and  the  6th  is  weak 
although  actually  present,*  is  manifestly  connected  with  the  structure  of  our  system 
of  musical  tones.  The  5th  and  6th  partial  tones  serve  to  delimit  the  minor 
Third,  and  in  this  way  almost  all  the  intervals  treated  as  consonances  in  modem 
music  are  determined  on  the  piano  by  coincident  upper  partials ;  the  Octave,  Fifth,  IT 
and  Fourth  by  relatively  loud  tones ;  the  major  Sixth  and  major  Third  by  weak 
ones;  and  the  minor  Third  by  the  weakest  of  all.  If  the  7th  partial  tone 
were  also  present,  the  subminorf  Seventh  4  :  7,  as  cf^h]),  would  injure  the  har- 
moniousness  of  the  minor  Sixth ;  the  Subminor  Fifth  5  :  7 ,  as  c'^fc'[>,  that  of  the  Fifth 
and  Fourth ;  and  the  subminor  Third  6  :  7,  as  g'''h'\},  that  of  the  minor  Third ;  with- 
out any  gain  in  the  more  accurate  determination  of  new  intervals  suitable  for 
musical  purposes. 

Mention  has  already  been  made  of  a  further  peculiarity  in  the* selection  of 
quahty  of  tone  on  the  pianoforte,  namely  that  its  upper  notes  have  fewer  and  weaker 
upper  partial  tones  than  the  lower.  This  difference  is  much  more  marked  on  the 
piano  than  on  any  other  instrument,  and  the  musical  reason  is  easily  assigned.  The 
high  notes  are  usuaUy  played  in  combination  with  much  lower  notes,  and  the 
relation  between  the  two  groups  of  notes  is  given  by  the  high  upper  partials  of  the 
deeper  tones.  When  the  interval  between  the  bass  and  treble  amounts  to  two  or  H 
three  Octaves,  the  second  Octave,  higher  Third  and  Fifth  of  the  bass  note,  are  in 
the  close  neighbourhood  of  the  treble,  and  form  direct  consonances  and  dissonances 
with  it,  without  any  necessity  for  using  the  upper  partials  of  the  treble  note. 
Hence  the  only  effect  of  upper  partials  on  the  highest  notes  of  the  pianoforte 
would  be  to  give  them  shrillness,  without  any  gain  in  respect  to  musical  definition. 
In  actual  practice  the  constmction  of  the  hammers  on  good  instruments  causes 
the  notes  of  the  highest  Octaves  to  be  only  gently  accompanied  by  their  second 
partials.  This  makes  them  mild  and  pleasant,  with  a  fiute-like  tone.  Some 
instrument-makers,  however,  prefer  to  make  these  notes  shrill  and  piercing,  like 
the  piccolo  flute,  by  transferring  the  striking  place  to  the  very  end  of  the  highest 
strings.     This  contrivance  succeeds  in  increasing  the  force  of  the  upper  partial 

*  [But  Bee  Mr.  Hipkins'  obsenrations  on      The  7th  partial  was  very  distinct  on  the  pianos 
PP-  17 y  78,  noie.— Translator.']  Mr,  Hipkins  examined.    See  also  App.  XX. 

t  [For  these  terms  see  ttie  table  on  p.  187.      Sect.  '^,— Translator,^ 

Digitized  by  Vj 00 QIC 


2IO    DISSONANCE  FOE  DIFFERENT  QUALITIES  OF  TONE,    paet  ii. 

tones,  but  gives   a   quality   of  tone  to  these  strings  which  does  not  suit  the 
ciiaracter  of  the  others,  and  hence  certainly  detracts  from  their  charm. 

In  many  other  instruments,  where  their  construction  does  not  admit  of  such 
absolute  control  over  the  quality  of  tone  as  on  the  pianoforte,  attempts  have  been 
made  to  produce  similar  varieties  of  quality  in  the  high  notes,  by  other  means. 
In  the  bowed  instruments  this  purpose  is  served  by  the  resonance  box,  the  proper 
tones  of  which  lie  within  the  deepest  Octaves  of  the  scale  of  the  instrument.  Since 
the  partial  tones  of  the  sounding  strings  are  reinforced  in  proportion  to  their 
proximity  to  the  partial  tones  of  the  resonance  box,  this  resonance  will  assist  the 
prime  tones  of  the  higher  notes,  as  contrasted  with  their  upper  partials,  much 
more  than  it  will  do  so  for  the  deep  notes.  On  the  contrary,  the  deepest  notes  of 
the  violin  will  have  not  only  their  prime  tones,  but  also  their  Octaves  and  Fifths 
favoured  by  the  resonance  ;  for  the  deeper  proper  tone  of  the  resonance  box 
%  hes  between  the  prime  and  2nd  partial,  and  its  higher  proper  tone  between 
the  2nd  and  3rd  partials.  A  similar  effect  is  attained  in  the  compound  stops  of 
the  organ,  by  making  the  series  of  upper  partial  tones,  which  are  represented  by 
distinct  pipes,  less  extensive  for  the  higher  than  for  the  lower  notes  in  the  stop. 
Thus  each  digital  opens  six  pipes  for  the  lower  octaves,  answering  to  the  first  six 
partial  tones  of  its  note ;  but  in  the  two  upper  octaves,  the  digital  opens  only  three 
or  even  two  pipes,  which  give  the  Octave  and  Twelfth,  or  merely  the  Octave,  in 
addition  to  the  prime. 

There  is  also  a  somewhat  similar  relation  in  the  human  voice,  although  it 
varies  much  for  the  different  vowels.  On  comparing  the  higher  and  lower  notes 
which  are  sung  to  the  same  vowel,  it  will  be  found  that  the  resonance  of  the  cavity 
of  the  mouth  generally  reinforces  relatively  high  upper  partials  of  the  deep  notes 
of  the  bass,  whereas  for  the  soprano,  where  the  note  sung  comes  near  to  the  charac- 
teristic pitch  of  the  vowel,  or  even  exceeds  it,  all  the  upper  partials  become  much 
%  weaker.  Hence  in  general,  at  least  for  the  open  vowels,  the  audible  upper  partials 
of  the  bass  are  much  more  numerous  than  those  of  the  soprano. 

We  have  still  to  consider  the  artificial  reed  instruments,  that  is  the  wind  in- 
struments of  wood  and  brass.  Among  the  former  the  clarinet,  among  the  latter 
the  horn  are  distinguished  for  the  softness  of  their  tones,  whereas  the  bassoon  and 
liautbois  in  the  first  class,  and  the  trombone  and  trumpet  in  the  second  represent 
the  most  penetrating  quahties  of  tone  used  in  music. 

Notwithstanding  that  the  keyed  horns  used  for  so-called  concerted  music  have 
a  far  less  braying  quality  of  tone  than  trumpets  proper,  which  have  no  side  holes, 
yet  the  number  and  the  force  of  their  upper  partial  tones  are  far  too  great  for  the 
harmonious  effect  of  the  less  perfect  consonances,  and  the  chords  on  these  instru- 
ments are  very  noisy  and  harsh,  so  that  they  are  only  endurable  in  the  open  air. 
In  artistic  orchestral  music,  therefore,  trumpets  and  trombones,  which  on  account 
of  their  penetrative  power  cannot  be  dispensed  with,  are  seldom  employed  for 
^  harmonies,  except  for  a  few  and  if  possible  perfect  consonances. 

The  clarinet  is  distinguished  from  all  other  orchestral  wind  instruments  by 
having  no  evenly  numbered  partial  tones.*  To  this  circumstance  must  be  due  many 
remarkable  deviations  in  the  effect  of  its  chords  from  those  of  other  instruments. 
When  two  clarinets  are  playing  together  all  of  the  consonant  intervals  will  be 
delimited  by  combinational  tones  alone,  except  the  major  Sixth  3:5,  and  the 
Twelfth  I  :  3.  But  the  differential  tones  of  the  first  order,  which  are  the  strongest 
among  all  combinational  tones,  will  always  suffice  to  produce  the  beats  of  imperfect 
consonances.  Hence  it  follows  that  in  general  the  consonances  of  two  clarinets 
have  but  little  definition,  and  must  be  proportionately  agreeable.  This  is  really  the 
case,  except  for  the  minor  Sixth  and  minor  Seventh,  which  are  too  near  the  major 
Sixth,  and  for  the  Eleventh  and  minor  Thirteenth,  which  are  too  near  the  Twelfth. 
On  the  other  hand,  when  a  clarinet  is  played  in  combination  with  a  violin  or  oboe, 
the  majority  of  consonances  will  have  a  perceptibly  different  effect  according  as 
*  [But  see  Mr.  Blaiklej's  observations,  supra,  p.  99&,  note.— Trarulafor.] 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


CHAPff*  XI.  XII.  THE  CONSONANT  CHORDS.  211 

the  clarinet  takes  the  upper  or  the  lower  note  of  the  chord.  Thus  the  major 
Third  d'  f%  will  sound  better  when  the  clarinet  takes  d'  and  the  oboe  f^,  so  that 
the  5th  partial  of  the  clarinet  coincides  with  the  4th  of  the  oboe.  The  3rd  and 
4th  and  the  5th  and  6th  partials,  which  are  so  disturbing  in  the  major  Third,* 
cannot  here  be  heard,  because  the  4th  and  6th  partials  do  not  exist  on  the  clarinet. 
But  if  the  oboe  takes  d'  and  the  clarinet  f%  the  coincident  4th  partial  will  be 
absent,  abd  the  disturbing  3rd  and  5th  present.  For  the  same  reason  it  follows 
that  the  Fourth  and  minor  Third  will  sound  better  when  the  clarinet  takes  the 
upper  tone.  I  have  made  experiments  of  this  kind  with  the  clarinet  and  a  bright 
stop  of  the  harmonium,  which  possessed  the  evenly  numbered  partial  tones,  and 
was  timed  in  just  intonation  f  and  not  in  equal  temperament.  When  ^  was  played 
on  the  clarinet,  and  e'|>,  d'y  d'\}y  in  succession  on  the  harmonium,  the  major 
Third  i[>  d'  sounded  better  than  the  Fourth  &[>  e'\},  and  much  better  than  the 
minor  llurd  h{}  dy.  If,  retaining  llj[}  on  the  clarinet,  I  played  /,  ^,  ^  in  succession  f 
on  the  harmonium,  the  major  Third  ^  &[>  was  rougher,  not  merely  than  the  Fourth 
/  6t>,  but  even  than  the  minor  Third  g  ^. 

This  example,  to  which  I  was  led  by  purely  theoretical  considerations  that 
were  immediately  confirmed  by  experiment,  will  serve  to  shew  how  the  use  of 
exceptional  quaHties  of  tone  will  affect  the  order  of  agreeableness  of  the  conso- 
nances which  was  settled  for  those  Usually  heard. 

Enough  has  been  said  to  shew  the  readiness  with  which  we  can  now  account 
for  numerous  peculiarities  in  the  effects  of  playing  different  musical  instruments 
in  combination.  Further  details  are  rendered  impossible  by  the  want  of  sufficient 
preliminary  investigations,  especially  into  the  exact  differences  of  various  qualities 
of  tone.  But  in  any  case  it  would  lead  us  too  far  &om  our  main  purpose  to  pursue 
a  subject  which  has  rather  a  technical  than  a  general  interest. 


CHAPTER  XII. 

GHOBDS. 

Wb  have  hitherto  examined  the  effect  of  sounding  together  only  two  tones  which 
form  a  determinate  interval.  It  is  now  easy  to  discover  what  will  happen  when 
more  than  two  tones  are  combined.  The  simultaneous  production  of  more  than 
two  separate  compound  tones  is  called  a  chord.  We  will  first  examine  the  har- 
moniousness  of  chords  in  the  same  sense  as  we  examined  the  harmoniousness  of 
any  two  tones  sounded  together.  That  is,  we  shall  in  this  section  deal  exclusively 
with  the  isolated  effect  of  the  chord  in  question,  quite  independently  of  any  musical 
connection,  mode,  key,  modulation,  and  so  on.  The  first  problem  is  to  determine 
under  what  conditions  chords  are  consonant,  in  which  case  they  are  termed  concords. 
It  is  quite  clear  that  the  first  condition  of  a  concord  is  that  each  tone  of  it  should  H 
form  a  consonance  with  each  of  the  other  tones ;  for  if  any  two  tones  formed  a 
dissonance,  beats  would  arise  destroying  the  tunefulness  of  the  chord.  Concords 
of  three  tones  are  readily  found  by  taking  two  consonant  intervals  to  any  one 
fundamental  tone  as  c,  and  then  seeing  whether  the  new  third  interval  between 
the  two  new  tones,  which  is  thus  produced,  is  also  consonant.  If  this  is  the  case 
each  one  of  the  three  tones  forms  a  consonant  interval  with  each  one  of  the  other 
two,  and  the  chord  is  consonant,  or  is  a  concord.^ 

Let  us  confine  ourselves  in  the  first  place  to  intervals  which  are  less  than  an 
Octave.  The  consonant  intervals  within  these  limits,  we  have  found  to  be :  i)  the 
Fifth  cg,i;  2)  the  Fourth  c/,  | ;  3)  the  major  Sixth  ca,^;  4)  the  major  Third 
ce,i;  s)  the  minor  Third  c  4>,  | ;  6)  the  minor  Sixth  c  at>,  f  ;  to  which  we  may 

•  [See  table  on  p.  191,  note,— Translator.]      third  are  dissonant  with  each  other,  I  call  the 
t  [Try  the   Harmonical    and   clarinet.—      result  a  '  con-dissonant  triad.*    See  App.  XX. 
Translator.]  eect.  E.  art.  5.— Transiator.! 

X  [If   two  tones  each  consonant  with  a  t 

Digitized  by  VjOQjSiC 


212 


THE  CONSONANT  CHOEDS. 


PABT  n. 


add  7)  the  snbminor  or  natural  Seventh  c'h^^,  J,  which  approaches  to  the  minor 
Sixth  in  harmoniousness.  The  following  table  gives  a  general  view  of  the  chords 
contained  within  an  Octave,  The  chord  is  supposed  to  consist  of  the  funda- 
mental tone  C,  some  one  tone  in  the  first  horizontal  line,  and  some  one  tone  of 
the  first  vertical  column.  Where  the  line  and  column  corresponding  to  these 
two  selected  tones  intersect,  is  the  name  of  the  interval  which  these  two  latter 
tones  form  with  each  other.  This  name  is  printed  in  italics  when  the  interval  ia 
consonant,  and  in  Boman  letters  when  dissonant,  so  that  the  eye  sees  at  a  glance 
what  concords  are  thus  produced.  [Under  the  name,  the  equivalent  interval  in 
cents  has  been  inserted  by  the  Translator.] 


c 

0 

Of 

702 

U 

884 

51 

Ebi 
316 

814 

6| 

702 

'J 

major 
Second 

I 
204 

u 

major 
Second 

major 
Third 

il 

minor 
Third 

minor 
Second 

if 
112 

Fourth 

4s 

316 

major 
Third 

major 
Second 

1 

Buperflnons 
Fourth 

minor 
Second 

1! 

70 

814 

minor 
Second 

112 

minor 
Third 

minor 
Second 

If 
70 

diminished 
Fourth 

& 

Fourth 

4. 

'Bbi 
969 

sabminor 
Third 

sab 
Fourth 

!i 
471 

BIlDZIllIlOr 

Second 

8 

snbminor 
Fifth 

sub 
Fifth 

& 

Bnbmajor 
Second 

15s 

H 


From  this  it  follows  that  the  only  consonant  triads  or  chords  of  three  notes, 
that  can  possibly  exist  within  the  compass  of  an  Octave  are  the  following : — 

i)    0  E  a  2)    C  E\}G 

3)    C  F  A  4)    C  F  AJ^ 

5)    GE\}A\^  6)    C  E  A.* 

The  two  first  of  these  triads  are  considered  in  musical  theory  as  the  funda- 
mental triads  from  which  all  others  are  deduced.  They  may  each  be  regarded  as 
composed  of  two  Thirds,  one  major  and  the  other  minor,  superimposed  in  different 
orders.  The  chord  C  E  Qyin  which  the  major  Third  is  below,  and  the  minor 
above,  is  a  major  triad.  It  is  distinguished  firom  all  other  major  triads  by  having 
its  tones  in  the  closest  position,  that  is,  forming  the  smallest  intervals  with  each 
other.  It  is  hence  considered  as  tiae  fundamental  chord  or  basis  of  all  other  major 
chords.  The  triad  GE^O,  which  has  the  minor  Third  below,  and  the  major  above, 
is  the  fundamental  chord  of  all  minor  triads, 

*  [The  reader  ought  to  hear  the  whole  set  nium,  organ,  and  piano  does  not  permit  this, 
of  triads  that  could  be  formed  from  the  table,  But  they  can  all  (inoludye  of  those  formed  by 
at  least  all  exclusive  of  those  formed  by  the  the  last  line)  be  played  on  the  Harmonioal. — 
last  line.    The  ordinary  tuning  of  the  harmo-      Tramlator.] 

Digitized  by  V^OOQIC 


OHAP.  xn.  DIFFERENCE  BETWEEN  MAJOE  AND  MINOB  CHORDS.  213 

The  next  two  ohords,  0  FA  and  0  F A\},  are  termed,  from  their  composition, 

chords  of  the  Sixth  and  Fomth,  written     [OioF  being  a  Fourth,  and  0  to  il 

a  major,  but  (7  to  il(>  a  minor  sixth].  If  we  take  O,  instead  of  0  for  the  funda- 
mental or  bass  tone,  these  chords  of  the  Fourth  and  Sixth  become  G,  G  E  and 
O,  C  Ej}.  Hence  we  may  conceive  them  as  having  been  formed  from  the  funda- 
mental major  and  minor  triads  CEO  and  C  JEr[>  (?,  by  transposing  the  Fifth  G  an 
Octave  lower,  when  it  becomes  G;. 

The  two  last  chords,  C  E\}A\^  and  0  E  A,eae  termed  chords  of  the  Sixth  and 

Third,  or  simply  chords  of  the  Sixth,  written     [OioE  being  a  major  Third,  and  G 

to  Ej}  aminor  Third;  and  G  to  A  ek  major  Sixth,  and  C  to  ii[>  a  minor  Sixth].    If 
we  take  JSr  as  the  bass  note  of  the  first,  and  Ej}  as  that  of  the  second,  they  become 
E  G  Ct  E\}Gc,  respectively.    Hence  they  may  be  considered  as  the  transpositions  H 
or  in/oersioTts  of  a  fundamental  major  and  a  fundamental  minor  chord,  G  E  G, 
C  Ej^Gfin  which  the  bass  note  G  is  transposed  an  Octave  higher  and  becomes  c. 

Collecting  these  Inversions,  the  six  consonant  triads  wiU  assume  the  following 
form  [the  numbers  shewing  their  correspondence  with  the  forms  on  p.  21 2d]  : — 

1)     G  E  G  2)    G  E\}  G 

5)         E  Gc  6)        Ej;}  Gc 

3)  Gee  4)  Gce^ 

We  must  observe  that  although  the  natural  or  subminor  seventh  ^J3[>  forms  a 
good  consonance  with  the  bass  note  0,  a  consonance  which  is  indeed  rather  superior 
than  inferior  to  the  minor  Sixth  G  A\},  yet  it  never  forms  part  of  any  triad,  because 
it  would  make  worse  consonances  with  all  the  other  intervals  consonant  to  0  than 
it  does  with  0  itself.  The  best  triads  which  it  can  produce  are  G  E''JB\}  =  4  :  5  :  7> 
and  0  G  '^j?  =  4:6:7.  In  the  first  of  these  occurs  the  interval  E  ''B\}  =  5  :  7,  H 
(between  a  Fourth  and  Fifth,)  in  the  latter  the  subminor  Third  G  ^-B^  =  6:7.* 
On  the  other  hand  the  minor  Sixth  makes  a  perfect  Fourth  with  the  minor  Third, 
so  that  this  minor  Sixth  remains  the  worst  interval  in  the  chords  of  the  Sixth  and 
Third,  and  of  the  Sixth  and  Fourth,  for  which  reason  these  triads  can  still  be  con- 
fddered  as  consonant.  This  is  the  reason  why  the  natural  or  subminor  Seventh  is 
never  used  as  a  consonance  in  harmony,  whereas  the  minor  Sixth  can  be  employed, 
although,  considered  independently,  it  is  not  more  harmonious  than  the  subminor 
Seventh. 

The  triad  G  E  A\},  to  which  we  shall  return,  [Chap.  XVII.  Dissonant  Triads, 
No.  4]  is  very  instructive  for  the  theory  of  music.  It  must  be  considered  as  a 
dissonance,  because  it  contains  the  diminished  Fourth  E  A\},  having  the  interval 
ratio  ff •  Now  this  diminished  Fourth  E  A^  is  so  nearly  the  same  as  a  major 
Third  E  G%,  that  on  our  keyed  instruments,  the  organ  and  pianoforte,  the  two 
intervals  are  not  distinguished.    We  have  in  fiust  ^ 

^^l>  =  t*  =  |.|if 
or,  approximatively  (S  A^  =  {E  G% .  Jf  t 

On  the  pianoforte  it  would  seem  as  if  this  triad,  which  for  practical  purposes  may 
be  written  either  G  E  A\}  or  G  E  Gj|l,  must  be  consonant,  since  each  one  of  its 
tones  forms  with  each  of  the  others  an  interval  which  is  considered  as  consonant 
on  the  piano,  and  yet  this  chord  is  one  of  the  harshest  dissonances,  as  all  musicians 
are  agreed,  and  as  any  one  can  convince  himself  immediately.  On  a  justly  intoned 
instrument  [as  the  Harmonica!]  the  interval  E  A\}  is  immediately  recognised  as 
dissonant.  This  chord  is  well  adapted  for  shewing  that  the  original  meaning  of 
the  intervals  asserts  itself  even  with  the  imperfect  tuning  of  the  piano,  and  deter- 
mines the  judgment  of  the  ear.$ 

*  [Add  the  consonance  G^B}dD  =  6  I  7  :  9,  cents,  difference  42  cents,  the  great  di&is. 
—Translator.]  See  App.  XX.  sect.  T>,— Translator,] 

i  [E  Ab  has  428  cents,  and  E  Oti  has  386  X  [Inserting  the  values  of  the  iotenrals  in 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQ IC 


C  E 

Q 

E  a 

c 

Q  C 

E 

C  E^ 

G 

E^Q 

0 

a  c 

Eb 

214    DIFFERENCE  BETWEEN  MAJOR  AND  MINOR  CHORDS,    pabt  n-- 

The  hannonious  effect  of  the  varioas  inversions  of  triads  already  found  depends 
in  the  first  place  upon  the  greater  or  less  perfection  of  the  consonance  of  the  several 
intervals  they  contain.  We  have  found  that  the  Fourth  is  less  agreeable  than  the 
Fifth,  and  that  minor  are  less  agreeable  than  major  Thirds  and  Sixths.  Now  the 
triad 

O  has  a  Fifth,  a  major  Third,  and  a  minor  Third 
a  Fourth,  a  minor  Third,  and  a  minor  Sixth 
a  Fourth,  a  major  Third,  and  a  major  Sixth 

a  Fifth,  a  minor  Third,  and  a  major  Third 
a  Fourth,  a  major  Third,  and  a  major  Sixth 
a  Fourth,  a  minor  Third,  and  a  minor  Sixth 

For  just  intervals  the  Thirds  and  Sixths  decidedly  disturb  the  general  tunefbl- 
^  ness  more  than  the  Fourths,  and  hence  the  major  chords  of  the  Sixth  and  Fourth 
are  more  harmonious  than  those  in  the  fundamental  position,  and  these  again 
than  the  chords  of  the  Sixth  and  Third.  On  the  other  hand  the  minor  chords  of 
the  Sixth  and  Third  are  more  agreeable  than  those  in  the  fundamental  position, 
and  these  again  are  better  than  the  minor  chords  of  the  Sixth  and  Fourth.  This 
conclusion  will  be  found  perfectly  correct  for  the  middle  parts  of  the  scale,  pro- 
vided the  intervals  are  all  justly  intoned.  The  chords  must  be  struck  separately, 
and  not  connected  by  any  modulation.  As  soon  as  modulational  connections 
are  allowed,  as  for  example  in  a  concluding  cadence,  the  tonic  feeling,  which  finds 
repose  in  the  tonic  chord,  disturbs  the  power  of  observation,  which  is  here  the 
point  of  importance.  In  the  lower  parts  of  the  scale  either  major  or  minor  Thirds 
are  more  disagreeable  than  Sixths. 

Judging  merely  from  the  intervals  we  should  expect  that  the  minor  triad 
C  B^Q  would  sound  as  well  as  the  major  C  E  Gj  e^s  each  has  a  Fifth,  a  major 
1  and  a  minor  Third.  This  is,  however,  far  from  being  the  case.  The  minor  triad 
is  very  decidedly  less  harmonious  than  the  major  triad,  in  consequence  of  the 
combinational  tones,  which  must  consequently  be  here  taken  into  consideration. 
In  treating  of  the  relative  harmoniousness  of  the  consonant  intervals  we  have  seen 
that  combinational  tones  may  produce  beats  when  two  intervals  are  compounded, 
even  when  each  interval  separately  produced  no  beats  at  all,  or  at  least  none 
distinctly  audible  (pp.  2oo2»-204&). 

Hence  we  must  determine  the  combinational  tones  of  the  major  and  minor 
triads.  We  shall  confine  ourselves  to  the  combinational  tones  of  the  first  order 
produced  by  the  primes  and  the  first  upper  partial  tones.  In  the  following 
examples  the  primes  are  marked  as  minims,  the  combinational  tones  resulting 
from  these  primes  are  represented  by  crotchets,  those  from  the  primes  and  first 
upper  partials  by  quavers  and  semiquavers.  A  downwards  sloping  line,  when 
placed  before  a  note,  shews  that  it  represents  a  tone  slightly  deeper  than  that 
H  of  the  note  in  the  scale  which  it  precedes. 

I.)  Major  Triads  with  their  Combinational  Tones  :* 

cents,  the  two  ohords,  A^b  386  C  386  JEr,  and  triads  does  not  apply  to  tempered  chords,  in 

C  386  ^1  386  O^  are  seen  to  be  identical,  none  of  which  are  any  of  the  intervals  purely 

but  when  the  first  is  inverted  to  C  386  Ei  consonant. — Translator,^ 

428^ 'bit  becomes  different  from  the  other.  *  [As  all  the  differentials  most  be  harmonics 

Both,  however,  remain  harshly  dissonant.    On  of  C  66,  if  we  represent  this  note  by  i,  the 

tempered  instruments  of  course  they  become  harmonics  and  hence  differentials  will  1^  be 

identical  C  400  E  400  OU  ,  C  400  E  400  A  b,  and  contained  in  the  series 
are  very  harsh.    The  definition  of  consonant 

I        2        3       4        5        6        7        8        9  10        II        12        13        14        15        16 

C       e       g       d       d       ^     ''Vb    d*      d'  e"       »/'       f      "a"     '6"b      ft"       C" 

First  CAonZ.— The  notes  wiU  then  be  4, 5, 6,  Second  CAonl.— Notes  5, 6,  8 ;  Octaves  10, 

represented  by  minims,  and  their  Octaves  8,      12, 16. 
JO,  12,  which  are  not  given  in  notes.  i)  Crotchets,  6-5  =  1,8-6=2,  8-5« 3. 

3-8=12-1 


i)  Crotchets,  5—4  =i 6-5 « I,  6— 4  =  2.  2)  Quavers,  10-8=12-10=2,  12-8=4. 

3)  Quavers,  12— ID  a  2,  8-5  »  3.  3)  Semiquavers,  12-5  =  7,  i6-6=io,(but 

audible  partial,)  16—5 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


3  - 

4)  Semiquavers,  12— 5 »=  7,  12-4 » 8.  this  is  also  an  audible  partial,)  16-5^11, 


CHAT.  zii.  DIFFEBENGE  BETWEEN  MAJOB  AND  MINOB  GHOBOS.  215 


^P 


^^  b^W—         ^        '>mm\rjr 


s 


-J. 


I 


f 


■'i-* 


^ 


2.)  Minor  Triads  with  their  Combinational  Tones  :* 


$ 


bb    J^  u"W- 


^   ,    ,,  ^^i/k^- 


m 


m 


-mm. 


^ 


^ 


J--t 


^i^ 


In  the  major  triads  the  combinational  tones  of  the  first  order,  and  even  the 
deeper  combinational  tones  of  the  second  order  (written  as  crotchets  and  quavers) 
are  merely  doubles  of  the  tones  of  the  triad  in  deeper  Octaves.  The  higher 
combinational  tones  of  the  second  order  (written  as  semiquavers)  are  extremely 
weak,  because,  other  conditions  being  the  same,  the  intensity  of  combinational 
tones  decreases  as  the  interval  between  the  generating  tones  increases,  with  which 
again  the  high  position  of  these  combinational  tones  is  connected.    I  have  always  f 

whioh  being  more  than  half  an  equal  Semi- 
tone (51  cents)  above  equally  tempered  /'  is 
represented  on  the  staff  as  a  flattened /'8  • 
Third    C^ord.— Notes  6,  8,   lo;   Octaves 

12,  l6,  20. 

i)  Crotchets,  io-8»8-6»2,  io-6»4. 


2)  Qoavers,  12— io»2,  12—8-4. 

3)  Semiquavers,  20— 6  » 14. 
How  far  these  higher  notes  marked  by 

semiquavers  are  effective,  except  possibly 
when  they  beat  with  each  other,  or  with  some 
partials  of  the  original  notes,  remains  to  be 

ia34S6789 
A„b    A,b    Eb    Ab      c       eb     ^gb    ab     bb 

18      20     21      22      24      25      26      27      28 

fb     c"     'd"b  "d^b   e"b     ^'      >«/'     f     Yb 

The  omitted  harmonics  are  not  ased  in  this 
investigation,  though  differentials  of  higher 
orders  occur  up  to  the  48th  harmonic. 

First  CAori.— Notes  10,  12,  15;  Octaves 
20,  24,  30. 

i)  Crotchets,  12— 10-2,  15— 1203,  15— 
10  «  5. 

2)  Quavers,  20-15-5,  20-12 » 8,  24-15 

3)  Semiquavers,  24-10-14,  30-12-18, 
30-10=20. 

Second  Chord, — Notes  12,  15,  20 ;  Octaves 
24,  30,  40. 

1)  Crotchets,  15-12  =  3,  20-15  =  5,  20- 
12=^8. 

2)  Quavers,  24-20  =  4,  24-15  =  9,  30-20 
-10. 

3)  Semiquavers,  30-12=18,  40-15  =  25, 
40-12  =  28. 


proved.— IZVarwZator.] 

*  [In  minor  chords  the  case  is  different. 
On  referring  to  the  list  of  harmonics  in  the 
last  note,  it  will  be  seen  that  the  only  minor 
chord  is  10,  12,  15  or  0"  &'  6",  and  this  is  the 
chord  upon  the  major  Third  above  the  third 
Octave  of  the  fundfunental.  Hence  in  the 
example  where  the  chord  taken  is  c'  e'b  gr' 
and  its  inversions,  the  harmonics  must  be 
formed  on  A„b  which  is  the  same  interval 
below  &.  The  list  of  harmonics  in  these 
examples  is  therefore 


10 


II        12 

"d'b      e'b 


13        14        15        16 

"/      Vb       ff       a!b 


30        32        33        39       40 
f     a"b     "a"b    »V"      d" 

Third  CAori.—Note8  15,  20,  24 ;  Octaves  ^ 
30,  40,  48. 

1)  Crotchets,  24-20-4,  20*15-5,  24— 

1S-9. 

2)  Quavers,  30^24-6,  30-20=10,  40- 
24=16. 

3)  Semiquavers,  40-1^-25,  48-20  =  28, 
48  —  i^  =  33.  This  I  have  nere  represented  as 
"a"b  because  it  is  the  Twelfth  above  "d'b,  but 
in  the  text  it  is  called  a  flattened  a"  because  it 
is  almost  the  one-sixth  of  C- 5  28.  In  fact  on 
the  Harmonical,  |  x  528  =  880,  and  A„b  would 
^}'Cf/='5*33-'26-3,  sothat  33  x  26-3 - 867*9 
vibrations.  The  interval  880  :  867*9  ^^  ^ 
cents,  and  hence  a"  is  more  than  a  comma  too 
sharp.  The  same  observation  applies  as  in 
the  last  footnote  regarding  the  audible  effect 
of  the  high  notes,  when  not  beating  with  each 
other,  or  with  audible  partiale.- 2Van32ator.] 


Digitized  by^OOQlC 


2i6     DIFFEEENCE  BETWEEN  MAJOE  AND  MINOE  CHOEDS.   pam  n. 

been  able  to  hear  the  deeper  combinational  tones  of  the  second  order,  written  as 
quavers,  when  the  tones  have  been  played  on  an  harmonium,  and  the  ear  was 
assisted  by  the  proper  resonators  :*  but  I  have  not  been  able  to  hear  those  written 
with  semiquavers.  They  have  been  added  merely  to  make  the  theory  complete. 
Perhaps  they  might  be  occasionally  heard  from  very  loud  musical  tones  having 
powerful  upper  partials.    But  they  may  be  certainly  neglected  in  all  ordinary 


For  the  minor  triads,  on  the  other  hand,  the  combinational  tones  of  the  first 
order,  which  are  easily  audible,  begin  to  disturb  the  harmonious  effect.  They  are 
not  near  enough  indeed  to  beat,  but  they  do  not  belong  to  the  harmony.  For  the 
fundamental  triad,  and  that  of  the  Sixth  and  Third  [the  two  first  chords],  these 
combinational  tones,  written  as  crotchets,  form  the  major  triad  A\}  C  ^,  and  for 
the  triad  of  the  Sixth  and  Fourth  [the  third  chord],  we  find  entirely  new  tones, 

iT  A\}9  Bj[}f  which  have  no  relations  with  the  originaJ  triad.t  The  combinational 
tones  of  the  second  order,  however  (written  as  quavers),  are  sometimes  partly 
above  and  generally  partly  below  the  prime  tones  of  the  triad,  but  so  near  to  them, 
that  beats  must  arise;  whereas  in  the  corresponding  major  triads  the  tones  of 
this  order  fit  perfectly  into  the  original  chord.  Thus  for  the  fundamental  minor 
triad  in  the  example,  &  e]}  g\  the  deeper  combinational  tones  of  the  second  order 
give  the  dissonances  a|7  ^  (/,  and  similarly  for  the  triad  of  the  Sixth  and  Third, 
e]}  g'  c".  And  for  the  triad  of  the  Sixth  and  Fourth  g^  c"  e"[>  we  find  the  disso- 
nances B^  &  and  g'  a]}.  This  disturbing  action  of  the  combinational  tones  on  the 
harmoniousness  of  minor  triads  is  certainly  too  slight  to  give  them  the  character 
of  dissonances,  but  they  produce  a  sensible  increase  of  roughness,  in  comparison 
with  the  effect  of  major  chords,  for  all  cases  where  just  intonation  is  employed, 
that  is,  where  the  mathematical  ratios  of  the  intervals  are  preserved.  In  the 
ordinary  tempered  intonation  of  our  keyed  instruments,  the  roughness  due  to  the 

^  combinational  tones  is  proportionably  less  marked,  because  of  the  much  greater 
roughness  due  to  the  imperfection  of  the  consonances.  Practically  I  attribute 
more  importance  to  the  influence  of  the  more  powerful  deep  combinational  tones 
of  the  first  order,  which,  without  increasing  the  roughness  of  the  chord,  introduce 
tones  entirely  foreign  to  it,  such  as  those  of  the  A\}  and  ^  major  triads  in  the  case 
of  the  C  minor  triads.  The  foreign  element  thus  introduced  into  the  minor  chord 
is  not  sufficiently  distinct  to  destroy  the  harmony,  but  it  is  enough  to  give  a 
mysterious,  obscure  effect  to  the  musical  character  and  meaning  of  these  chords, 
an  effect  for  which  the  hearer  is  unable  to  account,  because  the  weak  combinational 
tones  on  which  it  depends  are  concealed  by  other  and  louder  tones,  and  are  audible 
only  to  a  practised  ear.j:  Hence  minor  chords  are  especially  adapted  to  express 
mysterious  obscurity  or  harshness.f  F.  T.  Vischer,  in  his  Esthetics  (vol.  iii. 
§  772),  has  carefully  examined  this  character  of  the  minor  mode,  and  shewn  how 
it  suits  many  degrees  of  joyful  and  painful  excitement,  and  that  all  shades  of 

^  feeling  which  it  expresses  agree  in  being  to  some  extent '  veiled '  and  obscure. 

Every  minor  Third  and  every  Sixth  when  associated  with  its  principal  com- 

♦  [See  note  f  on  p.  i^jd,— Translator.']  were  chosen  because  the  first  Third  in  the 

f  [From  the  list  of  h&rmonics  on  p.  215c  fundamental  position  is  major  in  the  first  case 

it  will  be  seen  that  these  tones  occur  as  lower  and  minor  in  the  second.    In  German  the 

harmonics  of  the  tone  whence  the  minor  chords  terms  are  dur  and  moZZ,  that  is,  hard  and 

are  derived. — Translator.]  soft.]     It  is  well  known  that  the  names  dur 

X  [The  Author  is  of  course  always  speaking  and  moll  are  not  connected  with  the  hard  or 

of  chords  in  just  intonation.    When  tempered,  soft  character  of  the  pieces  of  music  written 

as  on  the  harmonium,  even  the  major  chords  in  these  modes,  but  are  historically  derived 

are  accompanied  by  unrelated  combinational  from  the  angular  form  of  Q  and  the  rounded 

tones,  sufficiently  dose  to  beat  and  sufficiently  form  of  b  i  which  were  the  B  durum  and  B 

loud  for  Scheibler  to  have  laid  down  a  rule  molle  of  the  medieval  musical  notation.    [The 

for  counting  the  beats  in  order  to  verify  the  probable  origin  of  the  forms  b  B  ^  5  is  ^ven 

correctness  of  the  tempered  tuning  (seep.  203(?).  from  observations  on  the  plates  in  Gaf  onus's 

But  still  the  different  effects  of  the  two  chords  Theoricum    Opus    Ilarmonicae    DtscipUnae, 

are  very  marked.— Translator.]  1480,  the  earliest  printed  book  on  music,  in  a 

§  [The  English  names  major  and  minor  footnote,  infri  p.  31 2(Z.    Translator,] 


Digitized  by  V^OOQIC 


4CHAP.  XII.  INVEBSIONS  OP  CHORDS.  aiy 

binational  tone,  becomes  at  onoe  a  major  chord.  C  is  the  combinational  tone  of 
the  minor  Third  ef  g^ ;  o  of  the  major  Sixth  g  e\  and  g  of  the  minor  Sixth.e'  c".* 
Since,  then,  these  dyads  naturally  produce  consonant  triads,  if  any  new  tone  is 
Bdded  which  does  not  suit  the  triads  thus  formed,  the  contradiction  is  necessarily 
sensible. 

Modem  harmonists  are  unwilling  to  acknowledge  that  the  minor  triad  is  less 
consonant  than  the  major.  They  have  probably  made  all  their  experiments  with 
tempered  instruments,  on  which,  indeed,  this  distinction  may  perhaps  be  allowed 
to  be  a  Httle  doubtful.  But  on  justly  intoned  instruments  f  and  with  a  moderately 
piercing  quality  of  tone,  the  difference  is  very  striking  and  cannot  be  denied.  The 
old  musicians,  too,  who  composed  exclusively  for  the  voice,  and  were  consequently 
not  driven  to  enfeeble  consonances  by  temperament,  shew  a  most  decided  feeling 
for  that  difference.  To  this  feeling  I  attribute  the  chief  reason  for  their  avoidance 
of  a  minor  chord  at  the  close.  The  medieval  composers  dowd  to  Sebastian  Bach  ^ 
used  for  their  closing  chords  either  exclusively  major  chords,  or  doubtful  chords 
without  the  Third ;  and  even  Handel  and  Mozart  occasionally  conclude  a  minor 
piece  of  music  with  a  major  chord.  Of  course  other  considerations,  besides  the 
degree  of  consonance,  have  great  weight  in  determining  the  final  chord,  such  as 
the  desire  to  mark  the  prevailing  tonic  or  key-note  with  distinctness,  for  which 
purpose  the  major  chord  is  decidedly  superior.    More  upon  this  in  Chapter  XY. 

After  having  examined  the  consonant  triads  which  lie  within  the  compass  of  an 
Octave,  we  proceed  to  those  with  wider  intervals.  We  have  found  in  general  that 
consonant  intervals  remain  consonant  when  one  of  their  tones  is  transposed  an 
Octave  or  two  higher  or  lower  at  pleasure,  although  such  transposition  has  some 
effect  on  its  degree  of  harmoniousness.  It  follows,  then,  that  in  all  the  consonant 
chords  which  we  have  hitherto  found,  any  one  of  the  tones  may  be  transposed 
some  Octaves  higher  or  lower  at  pleasure.  If  the  three  intervals  of  the  triad  were 
consonant  before,  they  will  remain  so  after  transposition.  We  have  already  seen  ^ 
how  the  chords  of  the  Sixth  and  Third,  and  of  the  Sixth  and  Fourth,  were  thus 
obtained  from  the  fundamental  form.  It  follows  further  that  when  larger  inter- 
vals are  admitted,  no  consonant  triads  can  exist  which  are  not  generated  by  the 
transposition  of  the  major  and  minor  triads.  Of  course  if  such  other  chords  could 
exist,  we  should  be  able  by  transposition  of  their  tones  to  bring  them  within  the 
compass  of  an  Octave,  and  we  should  thus  obtain  a  new  consonant  triad  within 
this  compass,  whereas  our  method  of  discovering  consonant  triads  enabled  us  to 
determine  every  one  that  could  lie  within  that  compass.  It  is  certainly  true  that 
slightly  dissonant  chords  which  lie  within  the  compass  of  an  Octave  are  sometimes 
rendered  smoother  by  transposing  one  of  their  tones.  Thus  the  chord  i  :  ^  :  |^,  or 
C,  ^^,  ^-B[>,  J  is  slightly  dissonant  in  consequence  of  the  interval  i  :  J ;  the 
interval  i  :  |^,  or  subminor  Seventh,  does  not  sound  worse  than  the  minor  Sixth  ; 
the  interval  ^  :  I  is  &  perfect  Fifth.  Now  transposing  the  tone  ^-Et),  an  Octave 
higher  to  ^4>>  ^^^  ^^^  transforming  the  chord  into  i  :  |^ :  ^,  we  obtain  i  :  ^  in  ^ 
place  of  I  :  ^,  and  this  is  much  smoother,  indeed  it  is  better  than  the  minor  Tenth 
of  our  minor  scale  i  .'  V)§  and  a  chord  thus  composed,  which  I  have  had  carefully 
tuned  on  the  harmonium,  although  its  unusual  intervals  produced  a  strange  effect, 
is  not  rougher  in  sound  than  the  worst  minor  chord,  that  of  the  Sixth  and  Fourth. 
This  chord,  C,  ^^,  ^e|7,  is  also  much  injured  by  the  unsuitable  combinational  tones 
Gf  and  F.**    Of  course  it  would  not  be  worth  while  to  introduce  such  strange 

*  (Tor  if  :  ^^5  :  6,  diif.  6— 5=»  i  or  C;  t  [See  these  intervals  examined  in  p.  195, 

g  :  6'  =  3  :  S,  difl.  S-3«-2  or  c;  e' :  c"=S  ;  8,  note  *.— Translator.] 

diff.  8-5  =  3  o'  g.— Translator,]  §  [The  intervals  6  :  7  =  y' :  '^b,  3  :  7  =  g 

t  See  Copter  XVI.  for  remarks  upon  {ast  :  '&'b,  and    $  :  I2  =  e:  ff'  can  be  tried  and 

and  tempered  intonation,  and  for  a  jastly  in-  compared  on  the  HarmonicaL — TranslatorJ] 

toned  instrument  suitable  for   such   ezperi-  **  [The  ratios  are  12  ;  21  :  28,  and  21  —  12 

ments.    [The  Harmonioal  can  also  be  used.  ^9,  but  9  :  I2»3  :  4,  hence  if  12  is  C,  9  is  O^ 

See  App.  XX.  sect.  F.  for  this  and  other  in-  Again  28-12-16,  12  :  16  =  3  :  4  and  hence 

struments.]  16  is  F.—Translator.] 

Digitized  by  V^OOQIC 


2l8 


INVERSIONS  OP  CHORDS. 


PABTII« 


tones  as  ^J9[7,  ^4>>  ^^  ^^  ^^^  ^^^  ^^  ^^^  ^^  &  chord  which  in  itself  is  not 
superior  to  the  worst  of  our  present  consonant  chords,  and  for  which  the  tones 
could  not  be  transposed  without  greatly  deteriorating  its  effect.* 

The  transposition  of  some  tones  in  a  consonant  triad,  for  the  purpose  of  widen- 
ing their  intervals,  affects  their  harmoniousness  in  the  first  place  by  changing  the 
intervals.  Major  Tenths,  as  we  found  in  Chapter  X.  p.  1956,  sound  better  than 
major  Thirds,  but  minor  Tenths  worse  than  minor  Thirds,  the  major  and  minor 
Thirteenth  worse  than  the  minor  Sixth  (p.  196a).  The  following  rule  embraces  ail 
the  cases : — Those  intervals  in  which  the  smaller  of  the  two  nmnbers  expressing  the 
ratios  of  the  pitch  mtmbers  is  even,  are  impbovbd  by  hamng  one  of  their  tones 
transposed  by  an  Octave,  because  the  numbers  expressing  the  ratio  are  thus 
diminished. 

The  Fifth       .        .2:3  beoomea  the  Twa/th     .       .    2  :    6  »  i  :  3 

I  The  major  Third    .4:5  „  mcyor  Tenth      .    4  :  10  =  2  :  5 

The  stibminor  Third   6:7  „  subminor  Tenth    6  :  14  »  3  :  7. 

Those  intervals  in  which  the  smaller  of  the  two  numbers  expressing  the  ratio  of 
the  vibrational  numbers  is  odd,  are  made  wobse  by  having  one  of  their  tones 
transposed  by  an  Octave,  as  the  Fourth  3  :  4  [which  becomes  the  Eleventh  3  :  8], 
the  minor  Third  5  :  6  [which  becomes  the  minor  Tenth  5:12],  and  the  Sixths 
[major]  3:5,  and  [minor]  5  :  8  [which  become  the  Thirteenths,  major  3  :  10  and 
minor  5  :  16]. 

Besides  this  the  principal  combinational  tones  are  of  essential  importance. 
An  example  of  the  first  combinational  tones  of  the  consonant  intervals  within 
the  compass  of  an  Octave  is  given  below,  the  primary  tones  being  represented 
by  minims  and  the  combinational  tones  by  crotchets,  as  before.f 


Iktbbyal.  Octave.  Donbl.  Oct    Fifth.      Twelfth.      Fourth.  Eleventh.  Maj.Third.  Magor  Tenth. 
Ratio.     4:8  2:8  ^  :6        4:  12  3:4        3:8  4:5  4:  10 

DiFnBBNGS.  4628  15  16 


Intebval.      Min.  Third.  Minor  Tenth.  Mcj.  Sixth.  Maj.  Thirteenth.  Min.  Sixth.  Min.  Thirteenth. 
Ratio.  5:6  S  :  12  3:5  3  '•  lo  5:8  S  :  16 

DiVFSBBNCB.  1727  3  II 

The  upwards  sloping  line  prefixed  to  /'  denotes  a  degree  of  sharpening  of  about 
a  quarter  of  a  Tone  [53  cents] ;  and  the  downwards  sloping  line  prefixed  to  61> 
flattens  it  [by  27  cents]  to  the  subminor  Seventh  of  c.    Below  the  notes  are  added 


*  [They  are,  however,  insisted  on  by  Poole, 
see  App.  XX.  sect.  F.  No.  e.-^TranslatorJ] 

t  [Some  of  the  bars  and  numbers  have  been 
changed  to  make  all  agree  with  the  footnote  to 


p.  21 4d.  All  these  notes  and  their  eombina- 
tional  notes  can  by  this  means  be  played  on 
the  Harmonical.— Tran32a/or.J 


Digitized  by 


Google 


CHAP.  xn. 


INVERSIONS  OF  CHORDS. 


919 


the  names  of  the  intervals,  the  numbers  of  the  ratios,  and  the  differences  of 
those  numbers,  giving  the  pitch  numbers  of  the  several  combinational  tones. 

We  find  in  the  first  place  that  the  combinational  tones  of  the  Octave,  Fifth, 
Twelfth,  Fourth,  and  major  Third  are  merely  transpositions  of  one  of  the  primary 
tones  by  one  or  more  Octaves,  and  therefore  introduce  no  foreign  tone.  Hence  these 
five  intervals  can  be  used  in  all  kinds  of  consonant  triads,  without  disturbing  the 
effect  by  the  combinational  tones  which  they  introduce.  In  this  respect  the  major 
Third  is  really  superior  to  the  major  Sixth  and  the  Tenth  in  the  construction  of 
chords,  although  its  independent  harmoniousness  is  inferior  to  that  of  either. 

The  double  Octave  introduces  the  Fifth  as  a  combinational  tone.  Hence  if  the 
fundamental  tone  of  a  chord  is  doubled  by  means  of  the  double  Octave,  the  chord 
is  not  injured.  But  injury  would  ensue  if  the  Third  or  Fifth  of  the  chord  were 
doubled  in  the  double  Octave. 

Then  we  have  a  series  of  intervals  which  are  made  into  complete  major  triads  f 
by  means  of  their  combinational  tones,  and  hence  produce  no  disturbance  in 
major  chords,  but  are  injurious  to  minor  chords.    These  are  the  Eleventh,  minor 
Third,  major  Tenth,  major  Sixth,  and  minor  Sixth, 

But  the  minor  Tenth,  and  the  major  and  minor  Thirteenth  cannot  form  part  of 
a  chord  without  injuring  its  consonance  by  their  combinational  tones. 

We  proceed  to  apply  these  considerations  to  the  construction  of  triads. 

I.  Majob  Tbiads. 

Major  triads  can  be  so  arranged  that  the  combinational  tones  remain  parts  of 
the  chord.  This  gives  the  most  perfectly  harmonious  positions  of  these  chords. 
To  find  them,  remember  that  no  minor  Tenths  and  no  [major  or  minor]  Thirteenths 
are  admissible,  so  that  the  minor  Thirds  and  [both  major  and  minor]  Sixths  must 
be  in  their  closest  position.  By  taking  as  the  uppermost  tone  first  the  Third,  then  % 
the  Fifth,  and  lastly  the  fundamental  tone,  we  find  the  following  positions  of  these 
chords,  within  a  compass  of  two  Octaves,  in  which  the  combinational  tones  (here 
written  as  crotchets  as  usual)  do  not  disturb  the  harmony. 


The  most  Perfect  Positiona  of  Major  Triads.* 
1234  56 


When  the  Third  lies  uppermost,  the  Fifth  must  not  be  more  than  a  major 
Sixth  below,  as  otherwise  a  [major]  Thirteenth  would  be  generated.  But  the  fun- 
damental tone  can  be  transposed.  Hence  when  the  Third  is  uppermost  the  only 
two  positions  which  are  undisturbed  are  Nos.  i  and  2.  When  the  Fifth  lies 
uppermost,  the  Third  must  be  inmiediately  under  it,  or  otherwise  a  minor  Tenth 


*  [CalcQlatioii  according  to  list  of  har* 
monies  p.  2i4d,  footnote. 

i)  Chord  4,  6,  10.  DifferentialB  6- 4  a  a, 
10— 6^4,  10-4^6,  which  is  also  one  of  the 
tones. 

2)  Chord  6,  8,  10.  Differentials  8-6 » 
10— 8b2,  10-6=4. 

3)  Chord  4,  10,  12.  Differentials  12-10 
■•2,  io~4  =  6,  i2-4=-8. 

4)  Chord  8,   10,  12.     Differentials  I0-8 


eI2-IO»2,  12  — 8-4. 

5)  Chord  3,  5,  8.  Differentials  5-3-2, 
8  —  5  =  3,  (which  is  also  one  of  the  tones,)  8—3 
a  5,  (which  is  also  one  of  the  tones). 

6)  Chord  5,  6,  8.  Differentials  6-5  =  1, 
8-6  =  2,8-5-3. 

These  chords  should  be  studied  on  the 
Harmonical,  and  the  combinational  tones  lis- 
tened for,  and  afterwards  the  tones  played  as 
substantive  notes.— 2Van^2a^.J 


Digitized  by 


\^oogle 


220 


INVEESIONS  OP  CHOEDS- 


PAET  n. 


would  be  produced ;  but  the  fundamental  tone  may  be  transposed.  Finallj,  when 
the  fundamental  tone  is  uppermost,  the  major  Third  can  lie  only  in  the  position  of 
a  minor  Sixth  below  it,  but  the  Fifth  maybe  placed  at  pleasure.  Hence  it  follows 
that  the  only  possible  positions  of  the  major  chord  which  will  be  entirely  free  firom 
disturbance  by  combinational  tones,  are  the  six  here  presented,  among  which  we 
find  the  three  chse  positions  Nos.  2,  4,  6  ahready  mentioned  [p.  215a],  and  three 
new  ones  Nos.  i,  3,  5.  Of  these  new  positions  two  (Nos.  i,  3)  have  the  funda- 
mental tone  in  the  bass,  just  as  in  the  primary  form,  and  are  considered  as  open 
positions  of  that  form,  while  the  third  (No.  5)  has  the  Fifth  in  the  bass,  just  as  in  the 
chord  of  the  Sixth  and  Fourth  [of  which  it  is  also  considered  as  an  open  position]. 
The  chord  of  the  Sixth  [and  Third]  (No.  6),  on  the  other  hand,  admits  of  no  opener 
position  [if  it  is  to  remain  perfectly  free  from  combinational  disturbance]. 

The  order  of  these  chords  in  respect  to  harmoniousness  of  the  intervals  is» 

^  perhaps,  the  same  as  that  presented  above.    The  three  intervals  of  No.  i  (the 

Fifth,  major  Tenth,  and  major  Sixth)  are  the  best,  and  those  of  No.  6  (the  Foorth, 

minor  Third,  and  minor  Sixth)  are  relatively  the  most  unfiavouiable  of  the 

intervals  that  occur  in  these  chords. 

The  remaining  positions  of  the  major  triads  present  individual  unsuitable  com- 
binational tones,  and  on  justly  intoned  instruments  are  unmistakably  rougher  than 
those  previously  considered,  but  this  does  not  make  them  dissonant,  it  merely  puts 
them  in  the  same  category  as  minor  chords.  We  obtain  all  of  them  which  lie 
within  the  compass  of  two  Octaves,  by  making  the  transpositions  forbidden  in  the 
last  cases.  They  are  as  follows,  in  tiie  same  order  as  before,  No.  7  being  made 
from  No.  i,  and  so  on : — 

The  less  Perfect  Positions  of  Major  Triads,"* 
89  10  II 


Musicians  will  immediately  perceive  that  these  positions  of  the  major  triad  are 
much  less  in  use.    The  combinational  tone  ^61>,  gives  the  positions  7  to  10  some- 


*  [Calculation  in  oontinaation  of  the  last 
note. 

7)  Chord  3,  4,  10.    DiflerentialB  4-3*1, 
io-4«6,  io-3a-7. 
%       8)  Chord  3,  8,  10.    Differentials  10-8-2, 
8-3«5,  10—3  =  7,  which  gives  the  interval 
7  :  8  with  the  tone  8. 

9)  Chord  4,  5,  12.  Differentials  5— 4^1, 
12— 4^8,  12—5  =  7,  the  two  last  differential 
tones  being  7  :  8. 

10)  Chord  5,  8,  1 2.  Differentials  8-5  =  3, 
12—804,  12  —  5  =  7,  which  gives  the  interval 
7  :  8  with  the  tone  8. 

11)  Chord  5,  6,  16.  Differentials  6-5=1, 
16-6=10,  16-5=11,  which  two  lastform  the 
dissonant  trumpet  interval  1 1  :  lo  of  165  cents 
or  about  three-quarters  of  an  equal  tone. 

12)  Chord  5,  12,  16.  Differentials  16-12 
»4,  12—5*7,  16—5  =  11,  which  forms  the 
same  dissonant  trumpet  interval  11  :  10,  but 
this  time  with  one  of  the  tones,  and  therefore 
more  harshly. 

AU  these  12  chords  should  be  well  studied 
on  the  Harmonical,  and  for  the  first  10,  the 


differential  tones  can  be  played  also  as  sub- 
stantive notes  (remembering  that  ^Bbison  the 
Ob  digital),  which  will  enable  the  student  to 
acquire  a  better  idea  of  the  roughness.  The 
tones  1 1  and  13  could  not  be  introduced  among 
the  first  4  Octaves  on  the  Harmonical  with- 
out incurring  the  important  losses  of  /'  and 
a".  But  if  we  take  the  chords  an  Oetave 
higher  we  can  play  "/"  and  >  V. 

The  chords  should  also  be  played  in  lower 
and  higher  positions,  not  only  as  Octaves  of 
those  given,  but  from  the  other  major  chords 
on  the  Harmonical  as  FA^Ct  OB^D,  A^bGJE^b^ 
E^b  OB^b.  Particular  attention  should  be 
paid  to  the  contrasting  of  the  positions  i  and  7, 
2  and  8,  3  and  9,  4  and  10,  5  and  11,  6  and 
12.  Unless  the  ear  acquires  the  habit  of 
attending  to  these  differences  it  wiU  not  pn>. 
perly  form  the  requisite  conceptions  of  major 
chords.  For  future  purposes  the  results  should 
also  be  contrasted  with  those  obtained  by  play* 
ing  the  same  chords  on  a  tempered  instrument, 
—if  possible  of  the  same  pitch,  A  44a — Trans- 
latoTi] 


Digitized  by 


Google 


OHAP.  xn. 


INVEESIONS  OF  CHOBDS. 


221 


thing  of  the  character  of  the  chord  of  the  dominant  Seventh  in  the  key  of  F 
major,  ceg}^.  The  two  last,  1 1  and  1 2,  are  much  the  least  pleasing ;  indeed  they 
are  decidedly  rougher  than  the  better  positions  of  the  minor  chord. 


2.  MiNOB  Tbiadb. 

No  minor  chord  can  be  obtained  perfectly  free  from  fiedse  combinational  tones, 
because  its  Third  can  never  be  so  placed  relatively  to  the  fundamental  tone,  as  not 
to  produce  a  combinational  tone  unsuitable  to  the  minor  chord.  If  only  one  such 
tone  is  admitted,  the  Third  and  Fifth  of  the  minor  chord  must  lie  close  together 
and  form  a  major  Third,  because  in  any  other  position  they  would  produce  a  second 
unsuitable  combinational  tone.  The  fundamental  tone  and  the  Fifth  must  never 
be  so  placed  as  to  form  an  Eleventh,  because  in  that  case  the  resulting  combina- 
tional tone  would  make  them  into  a  major  triad.  These  conditions  can  be  fulfilled  1[ 
by  only  three  positions  of  the  minor  chord,  as  follows  : — 


The  most  Perfect  Positions  of  Minor  Triads.* 
123 


1^ 


4=4 


^ 


^S 


t: 


The  remaining  positions  which  do  not  sound  so  well  are : — 


The  less  Perfect  Positions  of  Minor  Triads.f 
5  678  9  lo 

--1^^!t^._    ./^    |.J __isi. 


*  [Calculation  according  to  the  list  of  har- 
monies  on  p.  215c,  footnote. 

i)  Chord  24,  30,  40.  Dijfferentials  30-24 
»  6, 40— 30  s  10,  40— 24  B 16. 

2)  Chord  20,  24,30.  Differentials  24-20 
-4»  30-248-6,  30-20^10. 

3)  Chord  10,  24,  30.  Differentials  30-24 
-6,  24-io»i4,  30-10-20. 

These  can  also  be  stndied  on  the  Har- 
monical,  and  the  differentials  to  Nos.  i  and  2 
can  be  played  as  substantive  tones.  Not  so 
No.  3,  but  the  effect  may  be  felt  by  playing  the 
chord  a  major  Third  higher  as  eg'^,  being  the 
10,  24,  30  harmonics  of  C,.  and  giving  the  dif- 
ferentials O,  ^bb,  ef  which  can  be  played  as 
sabstantive  tones,  bnt  being  so  low  will  make 
the  effect  very  rough. — Translator,'] 

t  [Calculation  in  continuation  of  the  last 
note. 

4)  Chord  12,  15,  40.  Differentials  15-12 
«=3»  40- 15  =  25,  40-  12  =  28. 


5)  Chord  12,  30,  40.    Differentials  40-30 

» 10,  40-12=18,  40-12=28.  m 

6)  Chord  15,  20,  24.    Differentials  24-20  '' 
-4,20-15  =  5,24-15  =  9. 

7)  Chord  12,  20,  30.  Differentials  20-12 
»8,  30— 20»  10,  30— 12=  18,  where  18  forms 
the  dissonance  20  :  18=  10  :  9  with  the  tone 
20. 

8)  Chord  10,  15,24.  Differentials  15-10 
=  5,  24— 15  =  9,  24-10=14,  which  forms  the 
dissonant  interval  15  :  14  with  one  of  the  tones 

15- 

9)  Chord  10,  12,  30.  Differentials  12—10 
B  2,  30— 12  =  18, 30— 10=  20,  the  two  last  form 
together  the  dissonance  20  :  18=  10  :  9. 

10)  Chord  15, 20, 48  referred  to  A„,b .  In- 
terpret by  taking  the  Octaves  below  tne  num- 
bers in  p.  215c,  note.  Differentials  20—15  = 
5  =  C;  48-20  =  28=yb.  48- 15  =  33  =  "«'*», 
see  p.  2i$d\  note,  towards  the  end  of  the  ob- 
servations on  the  Third  Chord.  ^ 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQ IC 


222  INVEESIONS  OP  CHOEDS.  paetil 

The  positions  Nos.  4  to  lo  each  produce  two  unsuitable  combinational  tones, 
one  of  which  necessarily  results  from  the  fundamental  tone  and  its  [minor]  Third ; 
the  other  results  in  No.  4  from  the  Eleventh  O  C,  and  in  the  rest  horn  the  trans- 
posed major  Third  E^  G.  The  two  last  positions,  Nos.  11  and  12,  are  the  worst 
of  all,  because  they  give  rise  to  three  unsuitable  combinational  tones  [two  of  which 
beat  with  original  tones]. 

The  influence  of  the  combinational  tones  may  be  recognised  by  comparing  the 
different  positions.  Thus  the  position  No.  3,  with  a  minor  Tenth  c'  e"[>  and  major 
Third  e'^  g^',  sounds  unmistakably  better  than  the  position  No.  7,  with  major 
Tenth  e^  g"  and  major  Sixth  e^  c",  although  the  two  latter  intervals  when  struck 
separately  sound  better  than  the  two  first.  The  inferior  effect  of  chord  No.  7  is 
consequently  solely  due  to  the  second  unsuitable  combinational  tone,  b]}. 

This  influence  of  bad  combinational  tones  is  also  apparent  from  a  comparison 
f  with  the  major  chords.  On  comparing  the  minor  chords  Nos.  i  to  3,  each  of  which 
has  only  one  bad  combinational  tone,  with  the  major  chords  Nos.  11  and  12,  each 
of  which  has  two  such  tones,  those  minor  chords  will  be  found  really  pleasanter 
and  smoother  than  the  major.  Hence  in  these  two  classes  of  chords  it  is  not  the 
major  and  minor  Third,  nor  the  musical  mode,  which  decides  the  degree  of  harmo- 
niousness,  it  is  wholly  and  solely  the  combinational  tones. 

Four  Part  Chords  or  Tetrads. 

It  is  easily  seen  that  all  consonant  tetrads  must  be  either  major  or  minor  triads 
to  which  the  Octave  of  one  of  the  tones  has  been  added.*  For  every  consonant 
tetrad  must  admit  of  being  changed  into  a  consonant  triad  by  removing  one  of  its 
tones.  Now  this  can  be  done  in  four  ways,  so  that,  for  example,  the  tetrad  G  E  O  c 
gives  the  four  following  triads : — 

IF  C  E  G,        CEc,        E  Gc,        C  G  c. 

Any  such  triad,  if  it  is  not  merely  a  dyad,  or  interval  of  two  tones,  with  the 
Octave  of  one  added,  must  be  either  a  major  or  a  minor  triad,  because  there  are 
no  other  consonant  triads.  But  the  only  way  of  adding  a  fourth  tone  to  a  major 
or  minor  triad,  on  condition  that  the  result  should  be  consonant,  is  to  add  the 
Octave  of  one  of  its  tones.  For  every  such  triad  contains  two  tones,  say  G  and  G, 
which  form  either  a  direct  or  inverted  Fifth.  Now  the  only  tones  which  can  be 
combined  with  G  and  (?  so  as  to  form  a  consonance  are  E  and  E^;  there  are  no 
others  at  all.    But  E  and  E^  cannot  be  both  present  in  the  same  consonant  chord, 

11)  Chord  15,  40,  48  referred  to  ^^^b  ae  in  440  :  433*95  or  24  cents,  rather  more  than  a 

last  chord.  comma. 

Differentials   48 — 40  »  8 = il  b,  40  —  1 5  -  25  The  student  should  try  all  the  minor  chords 

se\  48-  i5  =  33-"a'b  as  in  last  chord,  which  not  only  in  different  positions  in  Octaves,  but 

Bee.  with  all  the  other  minor  chords  on  tibe  Har- 

U        12)  Chord   15,  24,  40  referred   to  A,„b>  monical,   namely,    FA^bC,    OB^bD,   D^FA^ 

Differentials  24— 15- 9  =  £b,  40— 24»i6»ab,  (which  contrast  with  the  dissonance   DFA^ 

40— 15  =  25  =  6' where  the  differentUls  16,  25  for  future  purposes),  AfiE^t  JS7,GB„  also  in 

form  the  dissonant  intervals  16  :  15,  25  :  24  different  Octaves,  till  the  ear  learns  to  distin- 

with  the  two  tones  15  and  24  respectively.    All  guish  these  12  different  forms, 
these  chords  can  be  studied  on  the  Harmonical,  Finally  the  12  forms  of  the  major  should 

and  their  differentials  can  be  played  as  sub-  be  contrasted  with  the  corresponding  12  forma 

stantive  tones  in  Nos.  6,  7,  and  12.    No.  8  can  of  the  minor  triad,  for  the  three  possible  cases 

be  taken  a  major  Third  higher  as  in  chord  FA^C    and   FA^bC;    CE^Q   and   CE^bG ; 

No.  3  of  the  last  note,  that  is  as  ef  b'  g"  giving  GB^D  and  GB'b-D.   To  merely  read  over  these 

the  differentials  e,  d,  'bb  which  can  be  played.  pages  by  eye  instead  of  studying  them  by  ear 

Also  No.  9  may  be  played  as  e'  g'  6"  giving  dif-  is  useless,  and  ordinary  tempered  instruments 

ferentials  c,  d",  e".    Nos.  4  and  5  do  not  admit  only  impede  instead  of  assisting  the  investi- 

of  such  treatment  because  e"'b  is  not  on  the  gator,— Translator,] 

instrument.  Nos.  10  and  1 1  cannot  be  so  played  *  [That  is,  if  we  exclude  the  harmonic 

because  "a'b  is  not  on  the  instrument.    In  Seventh  from  consideration,  as  on  p.  195^, 

fact  it  is  the  33rd  harmonic  of  il,,;b"  13*1 5t  and  those  who  admit  it  (as  Mr.  Poole,  App.  X.X. 

this  (see  footnote  p.  215^',  remarks  on  Third  sect.  F.  No.  6)  consider  CE^Q'Bb  to  be  a  per- 

CJujrdf)  -33  X  1 3*  15  «  433*95  vib. ;  whereas  a=  fectly  consonant  tetrad.— Trans totor.] 
440,  and  hence  is  too  sharp  by  the  interval 


Digitized  by 


^.joogle 


OHAP.  XII. 


INVERSIONS  OP  CHORDS- 


225 


and  hence  every  consonant  chord  of  four  or  more  parts,  which  contains  C  and  G, 
must  either  contain  E  and  some  of  the  Octaves  of  (7»  E,  G,  or  else  Ej[}  and  some 
of  the  Octaves  of  C,  ^,  O. 

Every  consonant  chord  of  three  or  more  parts  will  therefore  he  either  a  major 
or  a  minor  chord,  and  maj  be  formed  firom  the  fimdamental  position  of  the  major 
and  minor  triad,  by  transposing  or  adding  the  Octaves  above  or  below  some  or  all 
of  its  three  tones. 

To  obtain  the  perfectly  harmonious  positions  of  major  tetrads,  we  have  again 
to  be  careful  that  no  minor  Tenths  and  no  [major  or  minor]  Thirteenths  occur. 
Hence  the  Fifth  may  not  stand  more  than  a  minor  Third  above,  or  a  Sixth  below 
the  Third  of  the  chord ;  and  the  fundamental  tone  must  not  be  more  than  a 
Sixth  above  the  Third.  When  these  rules  are  carried  out,  the  avoidance  of  the 
minor  Thirteenths  is  effected  by  not  taking  the  double  Octave  of  the  Third  and 
Fifth.  These  rules  may  be  briefly  enunciated  as  follows  :  Those  major  chords  are  % 
most  harmonious  in  which  the  fundamental  tone  or  the  Fifth  does  not  Ue  more  than 
a  Sixth  above  the  Third,  or  the  Fifth  does  not  lie  more  than  a  Sixth  above  or 
below  it.  The  fundamental  tone,  on  the  other  hand,  may  be  as  far  below  the  Third 
as  we  please. 

The  corresponding  positions  of  the  major  tetrads  are  found  by  combining  any 
two  of  the  more  perfect  positions  of  the  major  triads  which  have  two  tones  in 
common,  as  follows,  where  the  lower  figures  refer  to  the  positions  of  the  major 
triads  ahready  given. 


The  most  Perfect  Positions  of  Major  Tetrads  within  the  Compass  of 

Two  Octaves,* 


a 


^ 


IS 


s 


m-^ 


Sf 


m 


& 


?2= 


S2I 


1+2   1+3   1+4   1  +  5   2  +  4   2  +  5   2  +  6   3  +  4  3  +  6  4  +  6  5  +  6 

We  see  that  chords  of  the  Sixth  and  Third  must  lie  quite  close,  as  No.  7  ;  t 
and  that  chords  of  the  Sixth  and  Fourth  t  must  not  have  a  compass  of  more  than 
an  Eleventh,  but  may  occur  in  all  the  three  positions  (Nos.  5,  6,  11)  in  which  it 
can  be  constructed  within  this  compass.  Chords  which  have  the  fundamental  tone 
in  the  bass  can  be  handled  most  freely. 

It  will  not  be  necessary  to  enumerate  the  less  perfect  positions  of  major  tetrads,  ^r 
They  cannot  have  more  than  two  unsuitable  combinational  tones,  as  in  the  1 2th 
position  of  the  major  triads,  p.  220c.    The  major  triads  of  C  can  only  have  the 
false  combinational  tones  marked  ^6(^  and  ^y,  [that  is,  with  pitch  numbers  bearing 
to  that  of  C  the  ratios  7  :  i,  or  11  :  i]. 

Minor  tetrads,  like  the  corresponding  triads,  must  at  least  have  one  fedse  com- 
binational tone.  There  is  only  one  single  position  of  the  minor  tetrad  which  has 
only  one  such  tone.  It  is  No.  i  in  the  following  example,  and  is  compounded  of 
the  positions  Nos.  i  and  2  of  the  minor  triads  on  p.  2216.    But  there  may  be  as 


*  [These  major  tetrads  can  all  be  played  on 
the  Harmonioal,  and  should  be  tried  in  every 
-position  of  Octaves  and  for  all  the  major  chords 
on  the  instrument,  namely  FA^C,  CE^Q, 
OBxD.A^bCE^b,  ^'bGB*b,  tiU  the  ear  is 
perfectly  familiar  with  the  different  forms  and 
the  student  can  tell  them  at  once  and  desig- 
nate them  by  their  number  in  this  list  on  hear- 


ing another  person  play  them. — Translator.] 
t  [This  chord  has  the  Third  both  lowest  and 

highest  and  is  marked     ,  but  is  more  com- 
monly marked  6. — Translator.] 

X  [These  chords  have  the  Fifth  lowest  and 

are  marked  ^. — Translator,] 
4 


Digitized  by 


Google 


224 


INVEB8I0N8  OP  CHORDS. 


PABT  H;) 


many  as  4  false  combinational  tones,  as,  for  example,  on  eombining  positicnis 
Nos.  10  and  11  of  the  minor  triads,  p.  221c. 

Here  follows  a  list  of  the  minor  tetrads  which  have  not  more  than  two  false 
combinational  tones,  and  which  lie  within  the  compass  of  two  Octaves.  The  Mse 
combinational  tones  only  are  noted  in  crotchets,  and  those  which  suit  the  chord- 
are  omitted. 

Best  Positions  of  Minor  Tetrads.* 

3  4  567" 


1  +  2 


1  +  3 


1  +  7 


2+3      2+6    2+7 


2  +  9 


3+S       1+6 


The  chord  of  the  Sixth  and  Fourth  [marked  n  occurs  only  in  its  closest  posi- 
tion, No.  5 ;  but  that  of  the  Sixth  and  Third  [marked     ]  is  found  in  three  positions 

(Nos.  3,  6,  and  9),  namely,  in  all  positions  where  the  compass  of  the  chord  does 
not  exceed  a  Tenth  ;  the  fundamental  chord  occurs  three  times  with  the  Octave  of 
the  fundamental  note  added  (Nos.  i,  2,  4),  and  twice  with  the  Octave  of  the  Fifth 
added  (Nos.  7,  8). 

In  musical  theory,  as  hitherto  expounded,  very  little  has  been  said  of  the 

IT  influence  of  the  transposition  of  chords  on  harmonious  efifect.  It  is  usual  to  give 
as  a  rule  that  close  intervals  must  not  be  used  in  the  bass,  and  that  the  intervals 
should  be  tolerably  evenly  distributed  between  the  extreme  tones.  And  even  these 
rules  do  not  appear  as  consequences  of  the  theoretical  views  and  laws  usually  given, 
according  to  which  a  consonant  interval  remains  consonant  in  whatever  part  of  the 
scale  it  is  taken,  and  however  it  may  be  inverted  or  combined  with  others.  They 
rather  appear  as  practical  exceptions  from  general  rules.  It  was  left  to  the 
musician  himself  to  obtain  some  insight  into  the  various  effects  of  the  various 
positions  of  chords  by  mere  use  and  experience.  No  rule  could  be  given  to  guide 
him. 

The  subject  has  been  treated  here  at  such  length  in  order  to  shew  that  a  right 
view  of  the  cause  of  consonance  and  dissonance  leads  to  rules  for  relations  which 

*    previous  theories  of  harmony  could  not  contain.    The  propositions  we  have  ennn- 


^         *  [Calcnlation  of  the  combinational  tones, 
by  the  list  of  harmonics  in  p.  215c. 

i)  Chord  20,  24,  30,  40.  Differentials 
24-2o  =  4  =  ilb,  40--24«  i6=sa'b. 

2)  Chord  10,  24,  30,  40.  Differentials 
24-10*  14  =  Vb»  40-24=  i6=a'b. 

3)  Chord  12,  15,  20,  30.  Differentials 
20-  i2  =  8«ab,  30~i2  =  i8s=6'b. 

4)  Chord  10,  20,  24,  30.  Differentials 
24-20  =  4  =ilb,  24-10=  I4«yb. 

5)  Chord  15,  20,  24,  30.  Differentials 
24-20  =  4  =  i4b,  24-i5«9=6b. 

6)  Chord  12,  20,  24,  30.  Differentials 
24-20=4a^b,  20-i2^8=ab,  30-12=18 
=  6'b. 

7)  Chord  10,  12,  15,  30.  Differentials 
i2-io=2-il,b,  30— I2  =  i8-6'b. 

8)  Chord  10,  15,  24,  30.  Differentials 
24-i5-9  =  bb,24-io«i4«yb. 

9)  Chord   12,   15,  20,  24.      Differentials 


24-20=4=ilb,    20-i2"8=ab,   24-15  =  9 
-6b. 

These  chords  should  all  be  studied  on  the 
Harmonical.  With  the  exception  of  Nos.  2,  4, 
7,  8  the  differentials  can  also  be  played  on  it 
as  sabstantive  tones.  Bnt  they  can  be  trans- 
posed. Thus  No.  2  may  be  played  as  «  (t'  2/  e" 
giving  the  differentials  ^&b,  </.  No.  4  will  be- 
come e^  ef'  g"  h"  giving  the  differential  'b'b, 
which  can  be  playeid.  No.  7  becomes  e'  ^  b'  b" 
giving  the  differentials  C  and  d".  No.  8  be- 
comes ef  b'  ^'  b"  giving  the  differentials  d*  and 
'&'b .  These  chords  ^onld  also  be  studied  in 
aU  the  minor  forms  on  the  Harmonical,  not 
only  in  different  Octaves,  but  on  all  the  minor 
chords  on  that  instrument,  viz.  D,  jP  ^1  „  ^ ,  C  JEr  „ 
JB,  G  B„  F  4>  bC,  C  iP  b  (?.  (?  B»  bD.  tiU  the  ear 
recognises  the  form,  and  the  student  can  name 
the  number  of  the  position  to  another  person's 
playing. — TnwMZafor.J 


Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


CHAP.  XII.  INVERSIONS  OF  CHOEDS.  225 

<siated  agree,  however,  with  the  practice  of  the  best  composers,  of  those,  I  mean, 
who  studied  vocal  music  principally,  before  the  great  development  of  instrumental 
music  necessitated  the  general  introduction  of  tempered  intonation,  as  any  one 
may  easily  convince  himself  by  examining  those  compositions  which  aimed  at 
producing  an  impression  of  perfect  harmoniousness.  Mozart  is  certainly  the  com- 
poser who  had  the  surest  instinct  for  the  dehcacies  of  his  art.  Among  his  vocal 
compositions  the  Ave  verum  corpus  is  particularly  celebrated  for  its  wonder- 
fully pure  and  smooth  harmonies.  On  examining  this  little  piece  as  one  of  the 
most  suitable  examples  for  our  purpose,  we  find  in  its  first  clause,  which  has 
an  extremely  soft  and  sweet  effect,  none  but  major  chords,  and  chords  of  the 
dominant  Seventh.  All  these  major  chords  belong  to  those  which  we  have  noted 
as  being  the  more  perfectly  harmonious.  Position  2  occurs  most  frequently,  and 
then  8,  10,  i,  and  9  [of  p.  223c].  It  is  not  till  we  come  to  the  final  modulation  of 
this  first  clause  that  we  meet  with  two  minor  chords,  and  a  major  chord  in  an  f 
tmfjEivourable  position.  It  is  very  striking,  by  way  of  comparison,  to  find  that  the 
second  clause  of  the  same  piece,  which  is  more  veiled,  longing,  and  mystical,  and 
laboriously  modulates  through  bolder  transitions  and  harsher  dissonances,  has 
many  more  minor  chords,  which,  as  well  as  the  major  chords  scattered  among 
them,  are  for  the  most  part  brought  into  unfavourable  positions,  until  the  final 
chord  again  restores  perfect  harmony. 

Precisely  similar  observations  may  be  made  on  those  choral  pieces  of  Palestrina, 
and  of  his  contemporaries  and  successors,  which  have  a  simple  harmonic  construc- 
tion without  any  involved  polyphony.  In  transforming  the  Roman  church  music, 
which  was  Palestrina's  task,  the  principal  weight  was  laid  on  harmonious  effect  in 
contrast  to  the  harsh  and  unintelhgible  polyphony  of  the  older  Netherland  *  system, 
and  Palestrina  and  his  school  have  really  solved  the  problem  in  the  most  pei^ect 
manner.  Here  also  we  find  an  almost  uninterrupted  fiow  of  consonant  chords,  with 
some  dominant  Sevenths,  or  dissonant  passing  notes,  charily  interspersed.  Here  f 
Also  the  consonant  chords  wholly,  or  almost  wholly,  consist  of  those  major  and 
minor  chords  which  we  have  noted  as  being  in  the  more  perfect  positions.  Only 
in  the  final  cadence  of  a  few  clauses,  on  the  contrary,  in  the  midst  of  more  powerful 
And  more  frequent  dissonances,  we  find  a  predominance  of  the  unfavourable  posi- 
tions of  the  major  and  minor  chords.  Thus  that  expression  which  modem  music 
endeavours  to  attain  by  various  discords  and  an  abundant  introduction  of  dominant 
Sevenths,  was  obtained  in  the  school  of  Palestrina  by  the  much  more  dehcate 
shading  of  various  inversions  and  positions  of  consonant. chords.  This  explains  the 
harmoniousness  of  these  compositions,  which  are  nevertheless  full  of  deep  and 
tender  expression,  and  sound  Uke  the  songs  of  angels  with  hearts  affected  but 
undarkened  by  human  grief  in  their  heavenly  joy.  Of  course  such  pieces  of  music 
require  fine  ears  both  in  singer  and  hearer,  to  let  the  delicate  gradation  of  expres- 
sion receive  its  due,  now  that  modem  music  has  accustomed  us  to  modes  of 
expression  so  much  more  violent  and  drastic.  ^ 

The  great  majority  of  major  tetrads  in  Palestrina's  Stabat  mater  are  in  the 
positions  i,  10,  8,  5,  3,  2,  4,  9  [of  p.  223c],  and  of  minor  tetrads  in  the  positions 
9,  2,  4,  3,  5,  I  [of  p.  224a].  For  the  major  chords  one  might  almost  think  that 
some  theoretical  rule  led  him  to  avoid  the  bad  intervals  of  the  minor  Tenth  and 
the  [major  or  minor]  Thirteenth.  But  this  rule  would  have  been  entirely  useless 
for  minor  chords.  Since  the  existence  of  combinational  tones  was  not  then 
known,  we  can  only  conclude  that  his  fine  ear  led  him  to  this  practice,  and  that 
the  judgment  of  that  ear  exactly  agreed  with  the  mles  deduced  from  our  theory. 

These  authorities  may  serve  to  lead  musicians  to  allow  the  correctness  of  my 
arrangement  of  consonant  chords  in  the  order  of  their  harmoniousness.  But 
any  one  can  convince  himself  of  their  correctness  on  any  justly  intoned  instrument 

*  [Including  both  the  modern  kingdom  of      1532,  was  born  in  Hainault  in  the  present 
the  Netherland(«.or  Holland,  and  the  still  more      Belgium.— Translator.] 
modem  kingdom  of  Belgium.    Josquin,  1450- 

Digitized  by  VjOOQIC 


2  26  KETEOSPECT.  part  n. 

(as  the  Hannonical].  The  present  system  of  tempered  intonation  certainly  oblite- 
rates somewhat  of  the  more  delicate  distinctions,  without,  however,  entirely 
destroying  them.  


Having  thus  concluded  that  part  of  our  investigations  which  rests  upon  purely 
BcientiQc  principles,  it  will  be  advisable  to  look  back  upon  the  road  we  have  travelled 
in  order  to  review  our  gains,  and  examine  the  relation  of  our  results  to  the  views  of 
older  theoreticians.  We  started  &om  the  acoustical  phenomena  of  upper  partial  tones, 
combinational  tones  and  beats.  These  phenomena  were  long  well  known  both  to 
musicians  and  acousticians,  and  the  laws  of  their  occurrence  were,  at  least  in  their 
essential  features,  correctly  recognised  and  enunciated.  We  had  only  to  pursue 
these  phenomena  into  further  detail  than  had  hitherto  been  done.  We  succeeded 
in  finding  methods  for  observing  upper  partial  tones^  which  rendered  comp^^tively 

f  easy  an  observation  previously  very  difficult  to  make.  And  with  the  help  of  this 
method  we  endeavoured  to  shew  that,  with  few  exceptions,  the  tones  of  all  musical 
instruments  were  compounded  of  partial  tones,  and  that,  in  especial,  those  qualities 
of  tone  which  are  more  particularly  f&vourable  for  musical  purposes,  possess  at 
least  a  series  of  the  lower  partial  tones  in  tolerable  force,  while  the  simple  tones, 
like  those  of  stopped  organ  pipes,  have  a  very  unsatisfactory  musical  effect, 
although  even  these  tones  when  loudly  sounded  are  accompanied  in  the  ear  itself 
by  some  weak  harmonic  upper  partials.  On  the  other  hand  we  found  that,  for  the 
better  musical  qualities  of  tone,  the  higher  partial  tones,  from  the  Seventh  onwards, 
must  be  weak,  as  otherwise  the  quahty,  and  every  combination  of  tones  would  be 
too  piercing.  In  reference  to  the  beats,  we  had  to  discover  what  became  of  them 
when  they  grew  quicker  and  quicker.  We  found  that  they  then  fell  into  that 
roughness  which  is  the  peculiar  character  of  dissonance.  The  transition  can  be 
effected  very  gradually,  and  observed  in  all  its  stages,  and  hence  it  is  apparent  to 

%  the  simplest  natural  observation  that  the  essence  of  dissonance  consists  merely*  in 
very  rapid  beats.  The  nerves  of  hearing  feel  these  rapid  beats  as  roiigh  and 
unpleasant,  because  every  intermittent  excitement  of  any  nervous  apparatus  affects 
us  more  powerfully  than  one  that  lasts  unaltered.  With  this  there  is  possibly 
associated  a  psychological  cause.  The  individual  pulses  of  tone  in  a  dissonant 
combination  give  us  certainly  the  same  impression  of  separate  pulses  as  slow  beats, 
although  we  are  unable  to  recognise  them  separately  and  count  them  ;  hence  they 
form  a  tangled  mass  of  tone,  which  cannot  be  analysed  into  its  constituents.  The 
cause  of  the  unpleasantness  of  dissonance  we  attribute  to  this  roughness  and 
entanglement.  The  meaning  of  this  distinction  may  be  thus  briefly  stated :  Con- 
sonance is  a  contintious,  dissonance  an  intermittent  sensation  of  tone.  Two  con- 
sonant tones  flow  on  quietly  side  by  side  in  an  undisturbed  stream ;  dissonant 
tones  cut  one  another  up  into  separate  pulses  of  tone.  This  description  of  the 
distinction  at  which  we  have  arrived  agrees  precisely  with  Euclid's  old  definition, 

f  '  Consonance  is  the  blending  of  a  higher  with  a  lower  tone.  Dissonance  is 
incapacity  to  mix,  when  two  tones  cannot  blend,  but  appear  rough  to  the  ear.'  f 

After  this  principle  had  been  once  established  there  was  nothing  further  to  do 
but  to  inquire  under  what  circumstances,  and  with  what  degree  of  strength,  beats 

*  [Bat  Bee  also  Prof.  Preyer,  in  App.  XX.  Fifth,  and  Fourth)  he  felt  that   the  tooBs 

sect.  L.  art.  7,  infra. — Tra/nslator,]  bUndecL    But  the  Sio^wWa  (which  he  applies 

t  EucUdeSt  ed.  Meibomius,  p.  8 :  lS<m  8^  to  all  other  intervals,  for  he  used  Pythagorean 
ffvfiipc»pta  fikp  Kpatris  S6o  ^6yy»Pf  h^vrdpov  Kot  major  and  minor  Thirds,  which  are  really  dis> 
fiafivT4pov.  Atai^yla  Sh  rohvarriov  S^fo  ^e6r/ymv  sonant)  he  found  to  consist  in  their  not  eTea 
hfu^ia,  m4  oimv  re  icpa^reu,  kKXh.  rpaxwBfiycu  mMng,  not  even  forming  a  mechanical,  mach 
rhy  &Ko^v.  [In  translating  this  passage  in  the  less  a  chemical  unit,  so  that  he  goes  on  to  ex- 
text,  I  have  endeavoured  to  make  the  distinc-  plain  that  this  non-mixing  of  the  two  tones 
tion  of  fi^is  and  tcpavu ;  the  former  is  taken  to  consisted  in  inability  to  blend,  and  resulted  in 
be  of  the  nature  of  a  mechanical,  and  the  producing  a  roughneast  as  contradistinguished 
latter  a  ehamical  mixture.  Mixing  and  blenui'  from  a  bUnding  in  the  ear.  The  tones  are 
ing  seem  to  convey  the  notion.  In  mtfi^via  ^6yyoi,  properly  tones  sung,  but  used  even  for 
(which  Euclid  admitted  only  for  the  Octave,  tones  of  the  lyre,— Translator.] 

Digitized  by  VjOOQ IC 


CHAP.  XII.  RETROSPECT.  227' 

would  arise  in  the  various  combinations  of  tones  through  either  the  partial  or  tlie 
combinational  tones.     This  investigation  had  hitherto  been  completely  worked  out 
by  Scheibler  for  the  combinational  tones  of  two  simple  tones  only.    The  law  of 
beats  being  known,  it  became  easy  to  extend  it  to  compound  tones.     Every 
theoretical  conclusion  on  this  field  can  be  immediately  checked  by  a  proper 
observation,  when  the  analysis  of  a  mass  of  tone  is  facilitated  by  the  use  ofi 
resonators.    All  these  beats  of  partial  and  combinational  tones,  of  which  so  much ) 
has  been  said  in  the  last  chapter,  are  not  inventions  of  empty  theoretical  specula- 
tion, but  rather  facts  of  observation,  and  can  be  reaUy  heard  without  difficulty  by  I 
any  practised  observer  who  performs  his  experiments  correctly.     The  knowledge 
of  the  acoustic  law  facilitates  our  discovery  of  the  phenomena  in  question.    But 
all  the  assertions  on  which  we  depend  for  establishing  a  theory  of  consonance  and 
dissonance,  such  as  was  given  in  the  last  chapters,  are  founded  wholly  and  solely 
on  a  careful  analysis  of  the  sensations  of  hearing,  an  analysis  which  a  practised  f 
ear  could  have  executed  without  any  theoretical  assistance,  although  of  course 
the  task  was  immensely  fe^^ilitated  by  the  guidance  of  theory  and  the  assistance  of 
appropriate  instruments  of  observation. 

For  these  reasons  the  reader  is  particularly  requested  to  observe  that  my  hypo- 
thesis concerning  the  sympathetic  vibration  of  Corti's  organs  inside  the  ear  has  no 
inmiediate  connection  whatever  with  the  explanation  of  consonance  and  dissonance. 
That  explanation  depends  solely  upon  observed  facts,  on  the  beats  of  partial  tones 
«nd  the  beats  of  combinational  tones.  Yet  I  thought  it  right  not  to  suppress  my 
hypothesis  (which  must  of  course  be  regarded  solely  as  an  hypothesis),  because  it 
gathers  all  the  various  acoustical  phenomena  with  which  we  are  concerned  into 
one  sheaf,  and  gives  a  clear,  intelligible,  and  evident  explanation  of  the  whole 
phenomena  and  their  connection. 

The  last  chapters  have  shewn,  that  a  correct  and  careful  analysis  of  a  mass  of 
sound  under  the  guidance  of  the  principles  cited,  leads  to  precisely  the  same  dis-  ^ 
tinctions  between  consonant  and  dissonant  intervals  and  chords,  as  have  been 
established  under  the  old  theory  of  harmony.  We  have  even  shewn  that  these 
investigations  give  more  particular  information  concerning  individual  intervals 
and  chords  than  was  possible  with  the  general  rules  of  the  former  theory,  and 
that  the  correctness  of  these  rules  is  corroborated  both  by  observation  on  justly 
intoned  instruments  and  the  practice  of  the  best  composers. 

Hence  I  do  not  hesitate  to  assert  that  the  preceding  investigations,  founded 
upon  a  more  exact  analysis  of  the  sensations  of  tone,  and  upon  purely  scientific, 
as  distinct  firom  esthetic  principles,  exhibit  the  true  and  sufficient  cause  of  conso« 
nance  and  dissonance  in  music. 

One  circumstance  may,  perhaps,  cause  the  musician  to  pause  in  accepting 
this  assertion.  We  have  found  that  firom  the  most  perfect  consonance  to  the 
most  decided  dissonance  there  is  a  continuous  series  of  degrees,  of  combinations  of 
sound,  which  continually  increase  in  roughness,  so  that  there  cannot  be  any  sharp  ^ 
line  drawn  between  consonance  and  dissonance,  and  the  distinction  would  therefore 
seem  to  be  merely  arbitrary.  Musicians,  on  the  contrary,  have  been  in  the  habit 
of  drawing  a  sharp  line  between  consonances  and  dissonances,  allowing  of  no 
intermediate  hnks,  and  Hauptmann  advances  this  as  a  principal  reason  against 
any  attempt  at  deducing  the  theory  of  consonance  from  the  relations  of  rational 
numbers.* 

As  a  matter  of  fEMst  we  have  already  remarked  that  the  chords  of  the  natural 

*  Harmomk  und  Meirik,  p.  4.    [At  the  the  sustained  tones  of  the  voice  for  example, 

same  time,  by  accepting  equal  tonperament  grossly  dissonant.    It  is  difficult  for  any  ear 

they  accept  as  consonant  a  series  of  tones  brougnt  op  among  these  dissonances,  to  under- 

which  really  form  only  one  consonant  interval  stand  the  real  distinction  between  consonance 

(the  Octave)  and  only  two  others  even  approzi-  and  dissonance.    Hence  the  absolute  necessity 

matively  consonant  (the  Fifth  and  Fourth),  of  testing  all  the  above  assertions  by  a  justly 

while  the  commonest  intervals  on  which  har-  intoned  instrument  such  as  the  Harmonical. — 

mony  rests,  the  Thirds,  with  their  inversions  Translator.] 
the  Sixths,  are  not  merely  dissonant  but,  on 


Digitized  by 


Google 


228  RETROSPECT.  part  lu 

or  subminor  Seventh  4  :  7  [c'  to  ''b]}  on  the  Harmonical],  and  of  the  subminor 
Tenth  3:7  [g  to  ^6'[>]  in  many  qualities  of  tone  sound  at  least  as  well  as  the 
minor  Sixth  5  :  8  [e'  to  c'^]^  and  that  the  subminor  Tenth  really  sounds  better 
than  the  ordinary  minor  Tenth  5:12  [«'  to  g^'].  But  we  have  already  noticed  a 
cLrcumstauce  of  great  importance  for  musical  practice  which  gives  the  minor  Sixth 
an  advantage  over  the  intervals  formed  with  the  number  7.  The  inversion  of  the 
minor  Sixth  gives  a  better  interval,  the  major  Third  [e^  c"  inverted  gives  c'  e'],  and 
its  importance  as  a  consonance  in  modem  music  is  especially  due  to  this  very 
relation  to  the  major  Third ;  it  isT  essentially  necessary^  and  justified,  just  because 
it  is  the  inversion  of  the  major  Third.  On  the  other  hand  the  inversion  or  trans- 
position of  an  interval  formed  with  the  number  7  leads  to  intervals  worse  than 
itself.  Hence,  as  it  is  necessary,  for  the  purposes  of  harmony,  to  have  the  power 
of  transposing  the  parts  at  pleasure,  we  have  a  sufficient  reason  for  drawing  the 

9  line  between  the  minor  Sixth  on  the  one  hand,  and  the  intervals  characterised 
by  7  on  the  other.  It  is  not,  however,  till  we  come  to  construct  scales,  which  we 
shall  have  to  consider  in  the  next  chapter,  that  we  find  decisive  reasons  for  making 
this  the  boundary.  The  scales  of  modem  music  cannot  possibly  accept  tones 
determined  by  the  number  7.*  But  in  musical  harmony  we  can  only  deal  with 
chords  formed  of  notes  in  the  scale.  Intervals  characterised  by  5,  as  the  Thirds 
and  Sixths,  occur  in  the  scale,  as  well  as  others  characterised  by  9,  as  the  major 
Second  8  :  9,  but  there  are  none  characterised  by  7,  which  should  form  the  tran- 
sition between  them.  Here,  then,  there  is  a  real  gap  in  the  series  of  chords  arranged 
according  to  the  degree  of  their  harmonious  effect,  and  this  gap  serves  to  determine 
the  boundary  between  consonance  and  dissonance. 

The  decision  does  not  depend,  then,  on  the  nature  of  the  intervals  themselves 
but  on  the  construction  of  the  whole  tonal  system.  This  is  corroborated  by  the 
fact  that  the  boundary  between  consonant  and  dissonant  intervals  has  not  been 

f  always  the  same.  It  has  been  already  mentioned  that  the  Greeks  always  repre* 
sented  Thirds  as  dissonant,  and  although  the  original  Pythagorean  Third  64  :  81, 
determined  by  a  series  of  Fifths,  was  not  a  consonance,  yet  even  when  the  natural 
major  Third  4  :  5  was  afterwards  included  in  the  so-called  syntono-diatonic  mode 
of  Didymus  and  Ptolemaeus,  it  was  not  recognised  as  a  consonance.  It  has 
already  been  mentioned  that  in  the  middle  ages,  first  the  Thirds  and  then  tlie 
Sixths  were  acknowledged  as  imperfect  consonances,  that  the  Thirds  were  long 
omitted  from  the  final  chord,  and  that  it  was  not  till  later  that  the  major,  and 
quite  recently  the  minor  Third  was  admitted  in  this  position.  It  is  quite  a  mis- 
take to  suppose,  with  modem  musical  theorists,  that  this  was  merely  whimsical 
and  unnatural,  or  that  the  older  composers  allowed  themselves  to  be  fettered  by 
blind  faith  in  Greek  authority.  The  last  was  certainly  partly  tme  for  writers  on 
musical  theory  down  to  the  sixteenth  century.  But  we  must  distinguish  carefully 
between  composers  and  theoreticians.    Neither  the  Greeks,  nor  the  great  musical 

m  composers  of  the  sixteenth  and  seventeenth  centuries,  were  people  to  be  blinded  by 

a  theory  which  their  ears  could  upset.     The  reason  for  these  deviations  is  to  be 

looked  for  rather  in  the  difference  between  the  tonal  systems  in  early  and  recent 

times,  with  which  we  shall  become  acquainted  in  the  next  part.    It  will  there  be 

seen  that  our  modem  system  gained  the  form  under  which  we  know  it  through  the 

influence  of  a  general  use  of  harmonic  chords.    It  was  only  in  this  system  that  a 

complete  regard  was  paid  to  all  the  requisitions  of  interwoven  harmonies.    Owiiig 

to  its  strict  consistency,  we  were  not  only  able  to  allow  many  licences  in  the  use 

of  the  more  imperfect  consonances  and  of  dissonances,  which  older  systems  had  to 

avoid,  but  we  were  often  required  to  insert  the  Thirds  in  final  chords,  as  a  mode 

of  distinguishing  with  certainty  between  the  major  and. minor  mode,  in  cases 

where  tliis  distinction  was  formerly  evaded. 

*  [Poole's  scale  /  9  a,  '5b  c'  d'  e\  /,  and  monio,  which  is  the  only  aconstical  jastiftcation 
Bosanquet's  and  White's  tempered  imitation  for  the  greatly  harsher  dominant  Seventh. — 
of  'fc'b,  properly  969  cents,  as  974  cents,  shew      Translator.'\ 


the  fdeluig  that  exists  for  using  the  7th  har- 


Digitized  by 


Google 


CHAP.  XII.  RETROSPECT,  329 

But  if  the  bonndary  between  consonance  and  dissonance  has  really  changed 
with  a  change  of  tonal  system,  it  is  manifest  that  the  reason  for  assigning  this 
boundary  does  not  depend  on  the  intervals  and  their  individual  musical  effect,  but 
on  the  whole  construction  of  the  tonal  system. 

The  enigma  which,  about  2500  years  ago,  Pythagoras  proposed  to  science, 
which  investigates  the  reasons  of  things,  '  Why  is  consonance  determined  by  the 
ratios  of  small  whole  numbers  ? '  has  been  solved  by  the  discovery  that  the  ear 
resolves  all  complex  sounds  into  pendular  oscillations,  according  to  the  laws  of 
sympathetic  vibration,  and  that  it  regards  as  harmonious  only  such  excitements  of 
the  nerves  as  continue  without  disturbance.  The  resolution  into  partial  tones, 
mathematically  expressed,  is  effected  by  Fourier's  law,  which  shews  how  any 
periodically  variable  magnitude,  whatever  be  its  nature,  can  be  expressed  by  a 
sum  of  the  simplest  periodic  magnitudes.*  The  length  of  the  periods  of  the 
simply  periodic  terms  of  this  sum  must  be  exactly  such,  that  either  one  or  two  ^ 
or  three  or  four,  and  so  on,  of  their  periods  are  equal  to  the  period  of  the 
given  magnitude.  This,  reduced  to  tones,  means  that  the  pitch  numbers  of  the 
partial  tones  must  be  exactly  once,  twice,  three  times,  four  times,  and  so  on, 
respectively,  as  great  as  that  of  the  prime  tone.  These  are  the  whole  numbers 
which  determine  the  ratios  of  the  consonances.  For,  as  we  have  seen,  the  con- 
dition of  consonance  is  that  two  of  the  lower  partial  tones  of  the  notes  combined 
shall  be  of  exactly  the  same  pitch ;  when  they  are  not,  disturbance  arises  from 
l^eats.  Ultimately,  then,  the  reason  of  the  rational  numerical  relations  of  Pytha- 
goras is  to  be  found  in  the  theorem  of  Fourier,  and  in  one  sense  this  theorem  may 
be  considered  as  the  prime  source  of  the  theory  of  harmony.f 

The  relation  of  whole  numbers  to  consonance  became  in  ancient  times,  in 
the  middle  ages,  and  especially  among  Oriental  nations,  the  foundation  of  extrava- 
gant and  fanciful  speculation.  '  Everything  is  Number  and  Harmony,'  was  the 
characteristic  principle  of  the  Pythagorean  doctrine.  The  same  numerical  ratios  ^ 
which  exist  between  the  seven  tones  of  the  diatonic  scale,  were  thought  to  be  found 
again  in  the  distances  of  the  celestial  bodies  from  the  central  fire.  Hence  the 
harmony  of  the  spheres,  which  was  heard  by  Pythagoras  alone  among  mortal  men, 
as  his  disciples  asserted.  The  numerical  speculations  of  the  Chinese  in  primitive 
times  reach  as  far.  In  the  book  of  Tso-kiu-ming,  a  friend  of  Confucius  (b.c.  500), 
the  five  tones  of  the  old  Chinese  scale  were  compared  with  the  five  elements  of 
their  natural  philosophy — ^water,  fire,  wood,  metal,  and  earth.  The  whole  numbers 
I,  2,  3  and  4  were  described  as  the  source  of  all  perfection.  At  a  later  time  the 
12  Semitones  of  the  Octave  were  connected  with  the  12  months  in  the  year,  and  so 
on.  Similar  references  of  musical  tones  to  the  elements,  the  temperaments,  and 
the  constellations  are  found  abundantly  scattered  among  the  musical  writings  of 
the  Arabs.  The  harmony  of  the  spheres  plays  a  great  part  throughout  the  middle 
ages.  According  to  Athanasius  Kircher,  not  only  the  macrocosm,  but  the  micro- 
cosm is  musical.  Even  Keppler,  a  man  of  the  deepest  scientific  spirit,  could  not  ^ 
keep  himself  free  from  imaginations  of  this  kind.  Nay,  even  in  the  most  recent 
times,  theorising  friends  of  music  may  be  found  who  will  rather  feast  on  arith- 
metical mysticism  than  endeavour  to  hear  upper  partial  tones. 

The  celebrated  mathematician  Leonard  EuleriJ:  tried,  in  a  more  serious  and 
more  scientific  manner,  to  found  the  relations  of  consonances  to  whole  numbers 
upon  psychological  considerations,  and  his  theory  may  certainly  be  regarded  as  the 
one  which  found  most  favour  with  scientific  investigators  during  the  last  century, 
although  it  perhaps  did  not  entirely  satisfy  them.  Euler  §  begins  by  explaining 
that  we  are  pleased  with  everything  in  which  we  can  detect  a  certain  amount  of 

*  Namely  magnitades  which  vary  as  sines  tance  by  Prof.  Preyer.    See  infra,  App.  XX. 

and  cosines.  sect  L.  art.  7. — TrafislatorJ] 

t  [The  coincidences  or  non-coincidences  of  %  Tentamen  ttovae  thcoriae  Mnaicae^  Petro- 

combinational  tones,  which  arc  independent  of  poli,  1739. 
Fourier's  law,  are  also  considered  of  impor-  §  hoc,  ciL  chap.  ii.  §  7. 

Digitized  by  VjOOQIC 


230  RETROSPECT.  pakt  ii, 

perfection.  Now  the  perfection  of  anything  is  determined  by  the  co-operation  of 
all  its  parts  towards  the  attainment  of  its  end.  Hence  it  follows  that  wherever 
perfection  is  to  be  found  there  must  be  order ;  for  order  consists  in  the  arrange- 
ment of  all  parts  by  a  certain  law  from  which  we  can  discover  why  each  part  lies 
where  it  is,  rather  than  in  any  other  place.  Now  in  any  perfect  object  such  a  law 
of  arrangement  is  determined  by  the  end  to  be  attained  which  governs  all  the 
parts.  For  this  reason  order  pleases  us  more  than  disorder.  Now  order  can  be 
perceived  in  two  ways :  either  we  know  the  law  whence  the  arrangement  is  de- 
duced, and  compare  the  deductions  from  this  law  with  the  arrangements  observed ; 
or,  we  observe  these  arrangements  and  endeavour  to  determine  the  law  from  them. 
The  latter  is  the  case  in  music.  A  combination  of  tones  will  please  us  when  we 
can  discover  the  law  of  their  arrangement.  Hence  it  may  well  happen  that  one 
hearer  finds  it  and  that  another  does  not,  and  that  their  judgments  consequently 

^  differ. 

The  more  easily  we  perceive  the  order  which  characterises  the  objects  contem* 
plated,  the  more  simple  and  more  perfect  will  they  appear,  and  the  more  easily  and 
joyfully  shall  we  acknowledge  them.  But  an  order  which  costs  trouble  to  discover, 
though  it  will  indeed  also  please  us,  will  associate  with  that  pleasure  a  certain 
degree  of  weariness  and  sadness  (tristitia). 

Now  in  tones  there  are  two  things  in  which  order  is  displayed,  pitch  and 
duration.  Pitch  is  ordered  by  intervals,  duration  by  rhythm.  Force  of  tone  might 
also  be  ordered,  had  we  a  measure  for  it.  Now  in  rhythm  two  or  three  or  four 
equally  long  notes  of  one  part  may  correspond  with  one  or  two  or  three  of  another, 
in  which  the  regularity  of  the  arrangement  is  easily  observed,  especially  when  fre- 
quently repeated,  and  gives  considerable  pleasure.  Similarly  in  intervals  we  should 
derive  more  pleasure  from  observing  that  two,  three,  or  four  vibrations  of  one  tone 
coincided  with  one,  two,  or  three  of  another,  than  we  could  possibly  experience  if 

f  the  ratios  of  the  time  of  vibration  were  incommensurable  with  one  another,  or  at 
least  could  not  be  expressed  except  by  very  high  numbers.  Hence  it  follows  that 
the  combination  of  two  tones  pleases  us  the  more,  the  smaller  the  two  numbers 
by  which  the  ratios  of  their  periods  of  vibration  can  be  expressed.  Euler  also 
remarked  that  we  could  better  endure  more  complicated  ratios  of  the  periods  of 
vibration,  and  consequently  less  perfect  consonances,  for  higher  than  for  deeper 
tones,  because  for  the  former  the  groups  of  vibrations  which  were  arranged  to 
occur  in  equal  times,  were  repeated  more  frequently  than  in  the  latter,  and  we 
were  consequently  better  able  to  recognise  the  regularity  of  even  a  more  involved 
arrangement. 

Hereupon  Euler  develops  an  arithmetical  rule  for  calculating  the  degree  of 
harmoniousness  of  an  interval  or  a  chord  from  the  ratios  of  the  periods  of  the 
vibrations  which  characterise  the  intervals.  The  Unison  belongs  to  the  first 
degree,  the  Octave  to  the  second,  the  Twelfth  and  Double  Octave  to  the  third,  the 

5[  Fifth  to  the  fourth,  the  Fourth  to  the  fifth,  the  major  Tenth  and  Eleventh  to  the 
sixth,  the  major  Sixth  and  major  Third  to  the  seventh,  the  minor  Sixth  and  minor 
Thvrd  to  the  eighth,  the  subminor  Seventh  4  :  7  to  the  ninth,  and  so  on.  To  the 
ninth  degree  belongs  also  the  major  triad,  both  in  its  closest  position  and  in  the 
position  of  the  Sixth  and  Fourth.  The  major  chord  of  the  Sixth  and  Third 
belongs,  however,  to  the  tenth  degree.  The  mn^nor  triad,  both  in  its  closest  and 
in  its  position  of  the  Sixth  and  Third,  also  belongs  to  the  ninth  degree,  but  its 
position  of  the  Sixth  and  Fourth  to  the  tenth  degree.  In  this  arrangement  the 
consequences  of  Euler 's  system  agree  tolerably  well  with  our  own  results,  except 
that  in  determining  the  relation  of  the  major  to  the  minor  triad,  the  infiuence  of 
combinational  tones  was  not  taken  into  a^M^ount,  but  only  the  kinds  of  interval. 
Hence  both  triads  in  their  close  position  appear  to  be  equally  harmonious,  although 
again  both  the  m>ajor  chord  of  the  Sixth  and  Third,  and  the  minor  chord  of  the 
Sixth  and  Fourth  are  inferior  with  him  as  with  us.* 

*  The  principle  on  "which  Euler  calculated  the   degrees  of  harmoniousneBB  for  interrala 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQIC 


CH.iP.  XII.  RETROSPECT.  231 

Enler  has  not  confined  these  speculations  to  smgle  consonances  and  chords,  bat 
has  extended  them  to  their  results,  to  the  construction  of  scales,  and  to  modula- 
tions, and  brought  out  many  surprising  specialities  correctly.    But  without  taking 
into  account  that  Euler's  system  gives  no  explanation  of  the  reason  why  a  conso- 
nance when  slightly  out  of  tune  sounds  almost  as  well  as  one  justly  tuned,  and  much 
better  than  one  greatly  out  of  tune,  although  the  numerical  ratio  for  the  former  iq 
generally  much  more  complicated,  it  is  very  evident  that  the  principal  difficulty  | 
in  Euler's  theory  is  that  it  says  nothing  at  all  of  the  mode  in  which  the  mind  con-  I 
trives  to  perceive  the  numerical  ratios  of  two  combined  tones.   We  must  not  forget  I 
that  a  man  left;  to  himself  is  scarcely  aware  that  a  tone  depends  upon  vibrations.  ^ 
Moreover,  immediate  and  conscious  perception  by  the  senses  has  no  means  of 
discovering  that  the  numbers  of  vibrations  performed  in  the  same  time  are  different, 
greater  for  high  than  for  low  tones,  and  that  determinate  intervals  have  deter- 
minate ratios  of  these  numbers.    There  are  certainly  many  perceptions  of  the  f 
senses  in  which  a  person  is  not  precisely  able  to  account  for  the  way  in  which  he 
has  attained  to  his  knowledge,  as  when  from  the  resonance  of  a  space  he  judges  of 
its  size  and  form,  or  when  he  reads  the  character  of  a  man  in  his  features.    But 
in  such  cases  a  person  has  generally  had  a  large  experience  in  such  relations,  which 
helps  him  to  form  a  judgment  in  analogous  circumstances,  without  having  the 
previous  circumstances  on  which  his  judgment  depends  clearly  present  to  his  mind. 
But  it  is  quite  different  with  pitch  numbers.    A  man  that  has  never  made  physical 
experiments  has  never  in  the  whole  course  of  his  life  had  the  slightest  opportunity 
of  knowing  anything  about  pitch  numbers  or  their  ratios.    And  almost  every  one 
who  delights  in  music  remains  in  this  state  of  ignorance  from  birth  to  death. 

Hence  it  would  certainly  be  necessary  to  shew  how  the  ratios  of  pitch  numbers 
can  be  perceived  by  the  senses.  It  has  been  my  endeavour  to  do  this,  and  hence 
the  results  of  my  investigation  may  be  said,  in  one  sense,  to  fill  up  the  gap  which 
Euler's  left.  But  the  physiological  processes  which  make  the  difference  sensible  ^ 
between  consonance  and  dissonance,  or,  in  Euler's  language,  orderly  and  disorderly 
relations  of  tone,  ultimately  bring  to  light  an  essential  difference  between  our 
method  of  explanation  and  Euler's.  According  to  the  latter,  the  human  mind 
perceives  commensurable  ratios  of  pitch  numbers  as  such  ;  according  to  our 
method,  it  perceives  only  the  physical  effect  of  these  ratios,  namely  the  continuous 
or  intermittent  sensation  of  the  auditory  nerves.*  The  physicist  knows,  indeed, 
that  the  reason  why  the  sensation  of  a  consonance  is  continuous  is  that  the  ratios 
of  its  pitch  numbers  are  commensurable,  but  when  a  man  who  is  unacquainted 
with  physics,  hears  a  piece  of  music,  nothing  of  the  sort  occurs  to  him,t  nor  does 
the  physicist  find  a  chord  in  any  respect  more  harmonious  because  he  is  better 
acquainted  with  the  cause  of  its  harmoniousness.^  It  is  quite  different  with  the 
order  of  rhythm.   That  exactly  two  crotchets,  or  three  in  a  triplet,  or  four  quavers 

and  chords,  is  here  annexed,  because  its  con-  becanse  60  is  the  least  common  multiple  of  m 

Bequences  are  very  correct,  if  combinational  4.  5i  6,  that  is,  the  least  number  which  all  of  ^ 

tones  are  disregarded.    When  p  is  a  prime  them  will  divide  without  a  remainder, 
number,  the  degree  is  =p.    All  other  numbers  *  [With  possibly  Prof.  Preyer's  addition, 

are  products  of  prime  numbers.    The  number  see  App.  XX.  sect.  L.  art.  7. — Translator.'] 
of  the  degree  for  a  product  of  two  factors  a  and  f  [Li  point  of  fact,  as  he  always  hears  tem- 

6,  for  which  separately  the  numbers  of  degree  pered  tones,  he  never  hears  the  exact  com- 

are  a  and  jS  respectively  »a+i8-i.    To  find  mensurable  ratios.    Indeed,  on  account  of  the 

the  number  of  the  degree  of  a  chord,  which  can  impossibility  of  tuning  with  perfect  exactness, 

be  expressed  by  p  :  g  :  r  :  «,  <&c.,  in  smallest  the  exact  ratios  are  probably  never  heard, 

whole  numbers,  Euler  finds  the  least  common  except  from  the  double  siren  and  wave-siren. — 

multiple  of  p^  9,  r,  s,  (fee,  and  the  number  of  Translator,'] 

its  degree  is  that  of  the  chord.    Thus,  for  :|:  [Does  a  man  breathe  more  easily  and 

example  :  aerate  his  blood  better  becanse  he  knows  the 

The  number  of  the  degree  of  2  is  2,  and  of  3  is  3,  J^^^*^*"*^?^  °?  ^^^^y'^T^^T  ""^^  '*^  ''^1**5'''' 

of    4=-  2x  2  it  is  2  +  2-1  =  1  to  his  carbonised  blood?    Does  a  man  feel  a 

of  12=  4  X  1*  it  is  -?  +  1  -  I  «  ?*  weight  greater  or  less,  because  he  knows  the 

of  60  =  12  X  ^*  it  is  ^  +  ^  - 1  =  Q  ^*^^  °'  gravitation  ?    These  are  quite  similar 

^'  ^     -^  ^'  questions.— rra»«?a/n»- 

That  of  the  major  triad  4  :  5  :  6  is  that  of  60, 


Digitized  by 


Google 


9^3  EETROSPECT.  -  pabt  n. 

go  to  one  minim  is  perceived  by  any  attentive  listener  without  the  least  instmotion. 
But  while  the  orderly  relation  (or  commensurable  ratio)  of  the  vibrations  of  two 
combined  tones,  on  the  other  hand,  undoubtedly  affects  the  ear  in  a  certain  way 
which  distinguishes  it  from  any  disorderly  relation  (incommensurable  ratio),  this 
difference  of  consonance  and  dissonance  depends  on  physical,  not  psychological 
grounds. 

The  considerations  advanced  by  Rameau*  and  d'Alembert  f  on  the  one  side,  and 
Tartini  t  on  the  other,  concerning  the  cause  of  consonance  agree  better  with  our 
theory.  The  last  founded  his  theory  on  the  existence  of  combinational  tones, 
the  two  first  on  that  of  upper  partial  tones.  As  we  see,  they  had  found  the 
proper  points  of  attack,  but  the  acoustical  knowledge  of  last  century  did  not  allow 
of  their  drawing  sufficient  consequences  from  them.  According  to  d'Alembert, 
Tartini's  book  was  so  darkly  and  obscurely  written  that  he,  as  well  as  other  well- 

^  instructed  people,  were  unable  to  form  a  judgment  upon  it.  D'Alembert's  book, 
on  the  other  hand,  is  an  extremely  clear  and  masterly  performance,  such  as  was 
to  be  expected  from  a  sharp  and  exact  thinker,  who  was  at  the  same  time  one  of 
the  greatest  physicists  and  mathematicians  of  his  time.  Bameau  and  d'Alembert 
lay  down  two  facts  as  the  foundation  of  their  system.  The  first  is  that  every 
resonant  body  audibly  produces  at  the  same  time  as  the  prime  (gMUrateur)  its 
Twelfth  and  next  higher  Third,  as  upper  partials  (harmoniqiies).  The  second  is 
that  the  resemblance  between  any  tone  and  its  Octave  is  generally  apparent.  The 
first  fact  is  used  to  shew  that  the  major  chord  is  the  most  natural  of  all  chords, 
and  the  second  to  establish  the  possibility  of  lowering  the  Fifth  and  the  Third  by 
one  or  two  Octaves  without  altering  the  nature  of  the  chord,  and  hence  to  obtain 
the  major  triad  in  all  its  different  inversions  and  positions.  The  minor  triad  is 
then  found  by  the  condition  that  all  three  tones  should  have  the  same  upper  partial 
or  harmonic,  namely  the  Fifth  of  tlie  chord  (in  fact  G,  J5t>,  and  G  have  all  the  same 

f  upper  partial  g')>  Hence  although  the  minor  chord  is  not  so  perfect  and  natural 
as  the  major,  it  is  nevertheless  prescribed  by  nature. 

In  the  middle  of  the  eighteenth  century,  when  much  suffering  arose  from  an 
artificial  social  condition,  it  may  have  been  enough  to  shew  that  a  thing  was 
natural,  in  order  at  the  same  time  to  prove  that  it  must  also  be  beautiful  and 
desirable.  Of  course  no  one  who  considers  the  great  perfection  and  suitability  of 
all  organic  arrangements  in  the  human  body,  would,  even  at  the  present  day,  deny 
that  when  the  existence  of  such  natural  relations  have  been  proved  as  Eameau 
discovered  between  the  tones  of  the  major  triad,  they  ought  to  be  most  carefully 
considered,  at  least  as  starting-points  for  further  research.  And  Bameau  had 
indeed  quite  correctly  conjectured,  as  we  can  now  perceive,  that  this  fact  was  the 

(proper  basis  of  a  theory  of  harmony.  But  that  is  by  no  means  everything.  For 
in  nature  we  find  not  only  beauty  but  ugliness,  not  only  help  but  hurt.  Hence  the 
mere  proof  that  anything  is  natural  does  not  suffice  to  justify  it  esthetically. 
in  Moreover  if  Bameau  had  listened  to  the  effects  of  striking  rods,  bells,  and  mem- 
branes, or  blowing  over  hollow  chambers,  he  might  have  heard  many  a  perfectly 
dissonant  chord.  And  yet  such  chords  cannot  but  be  considered  equally  natural. 
That  all  musical  instruments  exhibit  harmonic  upper  partials  depends  upon  the 
selection  of  qualities  of  tone  which  man  has  made  to  satisfy  the  requirements  of 
his  ear. 

Again  the  resemblance  of  the  Octave  to  its  fundamental  tone,  which  was  one 
of  Bameau 's  initial  facts,  is  a  musical  phenomenon  quite  as  much  in  need  of 
explanation  as  consonance  itself. 

No  one  knew  better  than  d'Alembert  himself  the  gaps  in  this  system.     Hence 

♦  [Trait4  de  Vliarmonic  r^duite  a  de^prirt'  1762. 

Hpes  iiaiurels,  1721.- Translator.]  J  [Trattato    di    Musica    secondo   la  vera 

t  &Ui)ients  de  Miisiqiie,  suivant  les  prin-  scieiiza  delV  anttotiia.    Padova,  175 1. — IZVoiw- 

cipes  de  M.  Rameau,  par  M.  d'Alembert.    Lyon,  Uitor.] 

Digitized  by  V^OOQIC 


CHAP.  XII.  EETEOSPECT.  233 

in  the  preface  to  bis  book  be  especially  guards  bimself  against  the  expression  : 
*  Demonstration  of  the  Principle  of  Harmony/  wbich  Eameau  had  used.  He 
declares  that  so  far  as  be  bimself  is  concerned,  be  meant  only  to  give  a  well- 
connected  and  consistent  account  of  all  the  laws  of  the  theory  of  harmony,  by 
deriving  them  from  a  single  fundamental  fact,  the  existence  of  upper  partial  tones 
or  harmonics,  which  he  assumes  as  given,  without  further  inquiry  respecting  its 
source.  He  consequently  limits  himself  to  proving  the  naturalness  of  the  major 
and  minor  triads.  In  bis  book  there  is  no  mention  of  beats,  and  hence  of  the 
real  source  of  distinction  between  consonance  and  dissonance.  Of  the  laws  of  beats 
very  little  indeed  was  known  at  that  time,  and  combinational  tones  had  only  been  just 
brought  under  the  notice  of  French  savants,  by  Tartini  (1751)  and  Romieu  (1753). 
They  had  been  discovered  a  few  years  previously  in  Germany  by  Sorge  (1745),  but 
the  fEhct  was  probably  little  known.  Hence  the  materials  were  wanting  for  build- 
ing up  a  more  perfect  theory.  V 

Nevertheless  this  attempt  of  Bameau  and  d'Alembert  is  historically  of  great  im- 
portance, in  so  fetr  as  the  theory  of  consonance  was  thus  for  the  first  time  shifted  from 
metaphysical  to  physical  ground.  It  is  astonisliing  what  these  two  thinkers  effected 
with  the  scanty  materials  at  their  command,  and  what  a  clear,  precise,  comprehensive 
system  the  old  vague  and  lumbering  theory  of  music  became  under  their  hands. 
The  important  progress  which  Bameau  made  in  the  specially  musical  portion  of 
the  theory  of  harmony  will  be  seen  hereafter. 

If,  then,  I  have  been  myself  able  to  present  something  more  complete,  I  owe  it 
merely  to  the  circumstance  that  I  had  at  command  a  large  mass  of  preliminary 
physical  results,  which  had  accumulated  in  the  century  that  has  since  elapsed. 


Digitized  by 


Google 


PART    III. 

THE  EELAHONSHIP  OF  MUSICAL  TONES. 


SCALES,  AND  TONALITY. 


CHAPTER  Xm. 

OfiNEBAL  VIEW  OF  THB  DIFFERENT  PBINCIPIiES  OF  MUSICAL  STYIiE   IN   THB 
DEVEIiOPMENT  OF  MUSIC. 

Up  to  this  point  our  investigation  has  been  purely  physical.  We  have  analysed 
the  sensations  of  hearing,  and  investigated  the  physical  and  physiological  caussB 
for  the  phenomena  discovered, — partial  tones,  combinational  tones,  and  beats.     Li 

%  the  whole  of  this  research  we  have  dealt  solely  with  natural  phenomena,  which 
present  themselves  mechanically,  without  any  choice,  to  all  living  beings  whose 
ears  are  constructed  on  the  same  anatomical  plan  as  our  own.  In  such  a  field, 
where  necessity  is  paramount  and  nothing  is  arbitrary,  science  is  rightfully  called 
upon  to  estabhsh  constant  laws  of  phenomena,  and  to  demonstrate  strictly  a  strict 
connection  between  cause  and  effect.  As  there  is  nothing  arbitrary  in  the  pheno- 
mena embraced  by  the  theory,  so  also  nothing  arbitrary  can  be  admitted  into  the 
laws  which  regulate  the  phenomena,  or  into  the  explanations  given  for  their  occur- 
rence. As  long  as  anything  arbitrary  remains  in  these  laws  and  explanations,  it  is 
the  duty  of  science  (a  duty  which  it  is  generally  able  to  discharge)  to  exclude  it,  by 
continuing  the  investigations. 

But  in  this  third  part  of  our  inquiry  into  the  theory  of  music  we  have  to  famish 
a  satisfactory  foundation  for  the  elementary  rules  of  musical  composition,  and  here 
we  tread  on  new  ground,  which  is  no  longer  subject  to  physical  laws  alone,  although 

%  the  knowledge  which  we  have  gained  of  the  nature  of  hearing,  will  still  find 
numerous  applications.  We  pass  on  to  a  problem  which  by  its  very  nature  belongs 
to  the  domain  of  esthetics.  When  we  spoke  previously,  in  the  theory  of  conso- 
nance, of  agreeable  and  disagreeable,  we  referred  solely  to  the  immediate  impression 
made  on  the  senses  when  an  isolated  combination  of  sounds  strikes  the  ear,  and 
paid  no  attention  at  all  to  artistic  contrasts  and  means  of  expression ;  we  thought 
only  of  sensuous  pleasure,  not  of  esthetic  beauty.  The  two  must  be  kept  strictly 
apart,  although  the  first  is  an  important  means  for  attaining  the  second. 

The  altered  nature  of  the  matters  now  to  be  treated  betrays  itself  by  a  purely 
external  characteristic.  At  every  step  we  encounter  historical  and  national  dif- 
ferences of  taste.  Whether  one  combination  is  rougher  or  smoother  than  another, 
dopends  solely  on  the  anatomical  structure  of  the  ear,  and  has  nothing  to  do  with 
psychological  motives.  But  what  degree  of  roughness  a  hearer  is  inclined  to 
endure  as  a  means  of  musical  expression  depends  on  taste  and  habit ;  hence  tlie 
boundary  between  consonances  and  dissonances  has  been  frequently  changed.   Simi- 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQIC 


CHAP.  XIII.  PHYSICAL  AND  ESTHETICAL  PRINCIPLES  COMPARED.    235 

larly  Scales,  Modes,  and  their  Modulations  have  undergone  multifarious  alterations, 
not  merely  among  uncultivated  or  savage  people,  but  even  in  those  periods  of  the 
world's  history  and  among  those  nations  where  the  noblest  flowers  of  human 
culture  have  expanded. 

Hence  it  follows, — and  the  proposition  is  not  even  now  sufficiently  present  to 
the  minds  of  our  musical  theoreticians  and  historians — that  the  system  of  Scales, 
Modes,  and  Harmonic  Tissues  does  not  rest  solely  upon  inalterable  natural  laws, 
hut  is  also,  at  least  partly j  the  result  of  esthetical  principles,  which  have  already 
changed,  and  will  still  further  change,  with  the  progressive  development  of  humanity. 

But  it  does  not  follow  irom  this  that  the  choice  of  those  elements  of  musical  art 
was  perfectly  arbitrary,  and  that  they  do  not  allow  of  being  derived  from  some 
more  general  law.  On  the  contrary  the  rules  of  any  style  of  art  form  a  well- 
connected  system  whenever  that  style  has  attained  a  full  and  perfect  development. 
These  rules  of  art  were  certainly  never  developed  into  a  system  by  the  artists  them-  ^ 
selves  with  conscious  intention  and  consistency.  They  are  rather  the  result  of  ten- 
tative exploration  or  the  play  of  imagination,  as  the  artists  think  out  or  execute 
their  plans,  and  by  trial  gradually  discover  what  kind  or  maimer  of  performance 
best  pleases  them.  Yet  science  can  endeavour  to  discover  the  motors,  whether 
psychological  or  technical,  which  have  been  at  work  in  this  artistic  process.  Scien- 
tific esthetics  have  to  deal  with  the  psychological  motor ;  scientific  physics  with 
the  technical.  When  the  artist's  aim  in  the  style  he  has  adopted,  and  its  prin- 
cipal direction,  have  once  been  rightly  conceived,  it  can  be  more  or  less  correctly 
determined  why  he  was  forced  to  follow  this  or  that  rule,  or  employ  this  or  that 
technical  means.  In  musical  theory,  namely  where  the  peculiar  physiological 
functions  of  the  ear,  while  not  immediately  present  to  conscious  self-examination, 
play  an  important  part,  a  large  and  rich  field  is  thrown  open  for  scientific  investi- 
gation to  shew  the  necessary  character  of  the  technical  rules  for  each  individual 
direction  in  the  development  of  our  art.  ^ 

It  does  not  rest  with  natural  science  to  characterise  the  chief  problem  worked 
out  by  each  school  of  art,  and  the  elementary  principle  of  its  style.  This  must  be 
gathered  from  the  results  of  historical  and  esthetical  inquiry. 

The  relation  we  liave  to  treat  may  be  illustrated  by  a  comparison  with  archi- 
tecture, which,  like  music,  has  pursued  essentially  different  directions  at  different 
times.  The  Greeks,  in  their  stone  temples,  imitated  the  original  wooden  construc- 
tions ;  that  was  the  principle  of  their  architectural  style.  The  whole  division  and 
arrangement  of  their  decorations  clearly  shew  that  it  was  their  intention  to  imitate 
wooden  constructions.  The  verticality  of  the  supporting  columns,  the  general 
horizontality  of  the  supported  beam,  forced  them  to  divide  all  the  subordinate  parts 
for  the  great  majority  of  cases  into  vertical  and  horizontal  lines.  The  purposes  of 
Grecian  worship,  which  performed  its  principal  functions  in  the  open  air,  were 
satisfied  by  erections,  of  this  kind,  in  which  the  internal  spaces  were  necessarily 
narrowly  limited  by  the  length  of  the  stone  or  wooden  beams  which  could  be  em-  ^ 
ployed.  The  old  Italians  (Etruscans),  on  the  other  hand,  discovered  the  principle 
of  the  arch,  composed  of  wedge-shaped  stones.  This  discovery  rendered  it  pos- 
sible to  cover  in  much  more  extensive  buildings  with  arched  roofs,  than  the  Greeks 
could  do  with  their  wooden  beams.  Among  these  arched  buildings  the  halls  of 
justice  (basil'icae)  became  important,  as  is  well  known,  for  the  subsequent  develop- 
ment of  architecture.  The  arched  roof  made  the  circular  arch  the  chief  principle 
in  division  and  decoration  for  Boman  {Byzantine)  art.  The  columns,  pressed  by 
heavy  weights,  were  transformed  into  pillars,  on  which,  after  the  style  was  fully 
developed,  columns  merely  appeared  in  diminished  forms,  half  sunk  in  the  mass  of 
the  pillar,  as  simply  decorative  articulations,  and  as  the  downward  continuation  of 
the  ribs  of  the  arches  which  radiated  towards  the  ceiling  from  the  upper  end  of  the 
pillar. 

In  the  arch  the  wedge-shaped  stones  press  against  each  other,  but  as  they  all 
uniformly  press  inwards,  each  one  prevents  the  other  from  falling.     The  most 

Digitized  by  V^OOQIC 


236      PHYSICAL  AND  ESTHETICAL  PRINCIPLES  COMPARED,  pabt  in. 

powerful  and  most  dangerous  degree  of  pressure  is  exerted  by  the  stones  in  the 
horizontal  parts  of  the  arch,  where  they  have  either  no  support  or  no  obliquely 
placed  support,  and  are  prevented  from  falling  solely  by  the  greater  thickness  of 
their  upper  extremities.  In  very  large  arches  the  horizontal  middle  portion  is  con- 
sequently the  most  dangerous,  and  would  be  precipitated  by  the  slightest  yielding 
of  the  materials.  As,  then,  medieval  ecclesiastical  structures  assumed  continu- 
ally larger  dimensions,  the  idea  occurred  of  leaving  out  the  middle  horizontal  part 
of  the  arch  altogether,  and  of  making  the  sides  ascend  with  moderate  obliquity 
until  they  met  in  a  pointed  arch.  From  thenceforward  the  pointed  arch  became 
the  dominant  principle.  The  building  was  divided  into  sections  externally  by  the 
projecting  buttresses.  These,  and  the  omnipresent  pointed  arch,  made  the  outlines 
hard,  and  the  churches  became  enormously  high.  But  both  characters  suited  the 
vigorous  minds  of  the  northern  nations,  and  perhaps  the  very  hardness  of  the  forms, 

"  thoroughly  subdued  by  that  marvellous  consistency  which  runs  through  the  varied 
magnificence  of  form  in  a  gothic  cathedral,  served  to  heighten  the  impression  of 
immensity  and  power. - 

We  see  here,  then,  how  the  technical  discoveries  which  were  associated  with 
the  problems  as  they  rose  successively  created  three  entirely  distinct  principles  of 
style — the  horizontal  line,  the  circular  arch,  the  pointed  arch — and  how  at  each 
new  change  in  the  main  plan  of  construction,  all  the  subordinate  individualities^ 
down  to  the  smallest  decorations,  were  altered  accordingly ;  and  hence  how  the 
individual  rules  of  construction  can  only  be  comprehended  from  the  general  prin- 
ciple of  construction.  Although  the  gothic  style  has  developed  the  richest,  the 
most  consistent,  the  mightiest  and  most  imposing  of  architectural  forms,  just  as 
modem  music  among  other  musical  styles,  no  one  would  certainly  for  a  moment 
think  of  asserting  that  the  pointed  arch  is  nature's  original  form  of  all  architectural 
beauty,  and  must  consequently  be  introduced  everywhere.    And  at  the  present  day 

^  it  is  well  known  that  it  is  an  artistic  absurdity  to  put  gothic  windows  in  a  Greek 
building.  Conversely  any  one  can  unfortunately  convince  himself  on  visiting  most 
of  our  gothic  cathedrals  how  detestably  unsuitable  to  the  whole  effect  are  those 
numerous  httle  chapels  of  the  renaissance  period  built  in  the  Greek  or  Roman  style. 
Just  as  little  as  the  gothic  pointed  arch,  should  our  diatonic  major  scale  be  regarded 
as  a  natural  product.  At  least  such  an  expression  is  quite  inapplicable,  except  in 
80  far  as  both  are  necessary  and  natural  consequences  of  the  principle  of  style 
selected.  And  just  as  little  as  we  should  use  gothic  ornamentation  in  a  Greek 
temple,  should  we  venture  upon  improving  compositions  written  in  ecclesiastical 
modes,  by  providing  their  notes  with  marks  of  sharps  and  flats  in  accordance  with 
the  scheme  of  our  major  and  minor  harmonies.  The  feeling  for  historical  artistic 
conception  has  certainly  made  little  progress  as  yet  among  our  musicians,  even 
among  those  who  are  at  the  same  time  musical  historians.  They  judge  old  music 
by  the  rules  of  modem  harmony,  and  are  inclined  to  consider  every  deviation  from 

H  it  as  mere  unskilfulness  in  the  old  composer,  or  even  as  barbarous  want  of 
taste.* 

Hence  before  we  proceed  to  the  construction  of  scales  and  rules  for  a  tissue  of 
harmony,  we  must  endeavour  to  characterise  the  principles  of  style,  at  least  for 
the  chief  phases  of  the  development  of  musical  art.  For  present  purposes  we  may 
divide  these  into  three  principal  periods  :-t- 

1.  The  Homophonic  or  Unison  Music  of  the  ancients,  to  which  also  belongs 
the  existing  music  of  Oriental  and  Asiatic  nations. 

2.  The  Polyphonic  Music  of  the  middle  ages,  with  several  parts,  but  without 
regard  to  any  independent  musical  significance  of  the  harmonies,  extending  firom 
the  tenth  to  the  seventeenth  century,  when  it  passes  into 

3.  Harmonic  or  Modern  Music,  characterised  by  the  independent  significance 

♦  Thus  in  R.  G.  Kiesewetter's  historico-  dently  an  exaggerated  zeal  to  deny  everything 
musical  writings,  which  are  otherwise  so  rich  which  will  not  fit  into  the  modern  major  and 
in   facts  industriously  collected,  there  is  evi-      minor  modes. 

Digitized  by  VjOOQlC 


CHAP.   XIII. 


PERIOD  OP  HOMOPHONIC  MUSIC. 


237 


attributed  to  the  harmonies  as  such.    Its  sources  date  back  from  the  sixteenth 
century. 

I.  HoMOFHONic  Music. 

One  part  music  is  the  original  form  of  music  with  all  people.  It  still  exists 
among  the  Chinese,  Indians,  Arabs,  Turks,  and  modem  Greeks,  notwithstanding 
the  greatly  developed  systems  of  music  possessed  by  some  of  these  nations.*  That 
music  in  the  time  of  highest  Grecian  culture,  neglecting  perhaps  individual  instru- 
mental ornamentation,  cadences,  and  interludes,  was  written  in  one  part,  or  that 
the  voices  at  most  sang  in  Octaves,  can  now  be  considered  as  established.  In  the 
problems  of  Aristotle  we  find  the  question :  *  Why  is  the  consonance  of  the  Octave 
alone  sung  ?  For  this  and  no  other  consonance  is  played  on  the  mag&dis.'  This 
was  a  harp-shaped  instrument  [with  a  bridge  dividing  the  strings  at  one-third 
their  length].  In  another  place  he  remarks  that  the  voices  of  boys  and  men  fom  f 
an  Octave  in  singing.f 

One  part  music,  considered  independently  and  unaccompanied  by  words,  is  too 
poor  in  forms  and  changes,  to  develop  any  of  the  greater  and  richer  forms  of  art. 
Hence  purely  instrumental  music  at  this  stage  is  necessarily  limited  to  short 
dances  or  marches.  We  really  find  no  more  among  nations  that  have  no  harmonic 
music4  Performers  on  the  flute  §  have  certainly  repeatedly  gained  the  prize  in 
the  Pythian  games,  but  it  is  possible  to  perform  feats  of  execution  in  instrumental 
music  in  concise  forms  of  composition,  as,  for  example,  in  the  variations  of  a 
short  melody.  That  the  principle  of  varying  (ficra^oXi;)  a  melody  with  reference 
to  dramatical  expression  (/u/ti;<ris),  was  known  to  tlie  Greeks,  follows  also  &om 
Aristotle.  He  describes  the  matter  very  plainly,  and  remarks  that  choruses  must 
simply  repeat  the  melodies  in  the  antistrophes,  because  it  is  easier  for  one  than 
for  several  to  introduce  variations.  But  public  competitors  (dyoivurrat)  and  actors 
(vroKpiTOL)  are  able  to  grapple  with  these  difficulties.**  ^ 

*  [See  App.  XX.  sect.  K.  for  some  of  these  the  effect  of  consonances  and  did  not  like  it, 
scales. — Translator.'] 

f  Aifl  tI  71  8iik  wcuT&¥  <rv/i<p<ovta  f  Srrai  fiSm/i ; 
fiayai't(ov<ri  yhp  rairriyj  ttWriy  9k  oh9efilay, 
Prob.  xix.  18.    flranslated  in  the  text.]     Ai^ 

rh  fuv  ikrrt^yoy  4rvfupvy6y  iori  8i2^  tnurAy ;  iK 
wtuBvy  ykp  yiw  kc^  h,vZp&y  ylyerai  rh  iani^yoy 
•1  Stcoraffi  rois  r6yotSf  &s  v^i?  trphs  r^f  irrdrfjy, 
Prob.  xix.  39.  [*  Why  is  a  consonant  union  of 
▼oices  pleasanter  than  a  single  voice  ?  Is  the 
singing  of  voice  against  voice,  a  consonant 
anion  of  voices  in  Octaves  ?  This  singing  of 
voice  against  voice  occurs  when  young  boys  and 
men  sing  together,  and  their  tones  differ  as  the 
highest  from  the  lowest  of  the  scale.']  Towards 
the  end  of  the  songs  the  instrumental  ac- 
companiment seems  to  have  separated  itself 
from  the  voice.  Probably  this  is  what  is  meant 
by  the  krouais  in  the  passage  rcAcvr»<ratf 
8'  «tf  TotfT^ir,  ly  Kcd  Kotyhy  rh  tpyov  avfu^aivu  yiy^ 
c9aiyKaBdw9prots  &whrify«p9riy  Kpoiovvi'  Kot 
yhp  oZrot  rit  &XAa  ob  irpo<ravXovrrct,  4ay  9ls  rav- 
rhy  Koeraorpi^wriy,  Mhippaiyovat  fuiWoy  r^  r4\9i 
l|  Kwowri  reus  wph  rov  r4\ovs  Sio^opcu;,  r^  rh 
4k  ^ta^6pwy  rh  KOtyhy,  liiurroy  in  rov  9ik  wcurAy 
yiytireai.  Arist  Prob.  xix.  39.  [*But  when 
they  end  in  the  same,  the  matter  is  precisely 
similar  to  what  occurs  when  they  play  an  ac- 
companiment to  a  song.  For  the  acoompany- 
ists  do  not  follow  the  rest,  but  when  the  singers 
return  to  the  same,  they  please  more  in  the  end, 
than  they  displease  in  the  differences  before 
the  end,  by  which  means  the  common  part  in 
what  is  generally  different,  pleases  more  than 
anything  but  the  Octave.*]  See  also  Plutarch, 
De  Muskar  i^x.  xxviii.    That  the  Greeks  knew 


-^V'^ 


appears  by  the  following  passage  from  Aristotle, 
De  A^idibilibus,  ed.  Beldcer,  p.  801 :  *  For  this 
reason  we  understand  a  single  speaker  better 
than  many  who  are  saying  the  same  things  at 
the  same  time.  And  so  with  strings.  And 
much  less,  when  flute  and  lyre  are  played  at 
the  same  time,  because  the  voices  are  confused 
by  the  others.  And  this  is  very  plain  for  the 
consonances.  For  both  tones  are  concealed, 
one  by  the  other.*  Ai^  koI  uaKXoy  Ms  Ajco^ktcs 
trvyiefxty^  ^  iroXAwy  &fAa  rairrii  \fy6yruy '  K<»0ir§p 
Kad  hr\  rAy  x^p^^^ '  *f«i  wo\h  ^rroy,  8ray  wpo<r- 
av\p  ris  ifM  kclL  Ki9api(ih  9tk  rh  trvyx*ifr9at  rks 
^cty^  vvb  rAy  kripnv.  Ovx  ffxiora  Zk  rovro  iiri 
r&y  <rvfjup»yi»y  i>av9p6y  itrriy.  *Afi<l>or4povs  7^ 
iiroKp^wrttrBai  cufAfiedytt  robs  ffx^M  vv*  iXXiiKuy. 

X  [In  Java  long  pieces  of  music  in  non-  m 
harmonic  scales  occur  to  accompany  actions 
and  develop  the  feeling  of  a  plot.    Many  instru- 
ments play  together,  but  there  is  no  harmony. 
— Translator.] 

§  The  airXoi  were  perhaps  more  like  our  oboes. 

**  [At&  rl  ol  /tky  y6fi4n  ovk  iy  iufTurrp6^iS 
iitoiovyro  *  ed  8i  (UXot  ^ctl  al  xopMoi  ;  *H  Uri  ol 
fiky  y6fA0t  kymvivrwv  Ijiray,  iy  Hitri  fiifuurdm 
Svyofity^y  Ked  ^tar^iyttrOcu,  ii  ^8j^  iylyero  poKpk 
ical  iroAu«i3^5;  xeiBdirtp  ody  koI  rh,  fii\ii  rp 
lufi-ficfti  iiKoKoMu  ktl  trtpa  yiy6fJLtyet,  MeiWoy 
yhp  r^  fi4K€i  ivdyicri  fjufAtlfrScUf  ^  rots  ^fuuri. 
At  h  Kcd  oi  9tB6p€^ifiotf  ivtt^^  fjufirirucol  iyiyoyro^ 
OVK  frt  ix^^^^^  kyrtarp^ovs,  wpirtpoy  84  cf^ov. 
hirtov  8c,  irt  rh  ira\euhy  oi  iKtvSfpot  4x^9*^^^ 
oJbroi.  TloKKovi  oiy  kywyiartKm  4^*ty^  x"^*^^*^ 
^y.  "SUrr*  4yap/jLuyta  fiaWoy  /ucAi)  4y^^y.  McTOr- 
fidWtty  yap  iroWhs  ^crajSuAar  t^^  iyl  paoy^  l| 
rots  voWots,   fcal    ry   aytoyiar^^  ^  roiS  rh  i^vs 

Digitized  by  V^OOQIC 


238 


PERIOD  OF  HOMOPHONIC  MUSIC. 


PART    III. 


Extensive  works  of  art,  in  homophonic  music,  are  only  possible  in  connection 
with  poetry,  and  this  was  also  the  way  in  which  music  was  applied  in  classical 
antiquity.  Not  only  were  songs  (odes)  and  religious  hymns  sung,  but  even 
tragedies  and  long  epic  poems  were  performed  in  some  musical  manner,  and 
accompanied  by  the  lyre.  We  are  scarcely  in  a  condition  to  form  a  conception  of 
how  this  was  done,  because  modem  taste  points  in  precisely  the  opposite  direction, 

,  and  demands  from  a  great  declaimer  or  public  reader  that  he  should  produce  a 
dramatic  effect  true  to  nature  by  the  speaking  voice  alone,  rating  all  approach  to 
singing  as  one  of  the  greatest  of  faults.  Perhaps  we  have  some  echoes  of  the 
ancient  spoken  song  in  the  singing  tone  of  Italian  declaimers,  and  the  liturgical 
recitations  (intoning)  of  the  Eoman  Catholic  priests.  Indeed,  attentive  observa- 
tions on  ordinary  conversation  shews  us  that  regular  musical  intervals  involun- 
tarily recur,  although  the  singing  tone  of  the  voice  is  concealed  under  the  noises 

f  which  characterise  the  individual  letters,  and  the  pitch  is  not  held  firmly,  but  is 
frequently  allowed  to  glide  up  and  down.  When  simple  sentences  are  spoken 
without  being  affected  by  feeling,  a  certain  middle  pitch  is  maintained,  and  it  is 
only  the  emphatic  words  and  the  conclusions  of  sentences  and  clauses  which  are 
indicated  by  change  of  pitch.*  The  end  of  an  aflSrmative  sentence  followed  by  a 
pause,  is  usually  marked  by  the  voice  falling  a  Fourth  from  the  middle  pitch.  An 
interrogative  ending  rises,  often  as  much  as  a  Fifth  above  the  middle  pitch.  For 
example  a  bass  voice  would  say : 


m 


^ 


3^ 


Ich       Hn       spa  -  tzie   -   rew      ge  -   gan  -  ^en. 
I        have     been  walk  -  ing     this   mom  -  ing. 


1 


^|c==-t 


Bist       du        spa  -  tzie  -   ren       ge  -  gam,   -  genf 
Have     you      been  walk  -  ing     this  mom  -  mg? 

Emphasised  words  are  also  rendered  prominent  by  their  being  spoken  about  a  Tone 
higher  than  the  rest,t  and  so  on.  In  solemn  declamation  the  alterations  of  pitch 
are  more  numerous  and  complicated.  Modem  recitative  has  arisen  from  attempt- 
ing to  imitate  these  alterations  of  pitch  by  musical  notes.  Its  inventor,  Giaconio 
Peri,  in  the  preface  to  his  opera  of  Eurydice,  published  in  i6oo,  distinctly  says  as 
much.  An  attempt  was  then  made  to  restore  the  declamation  of  ancient  tragedies 
by  means  of  recitative.  Ancient  recitative  certainly  differed  somewhat  from 
modem  recitative,  by  preserving  the  metre  of  the  poems  more  exactly,  and  by 


^vXdrrov^i.  At*  h  iarXo6<rrfpn  iwoiovvro  aibrois 
m  rdfi«Aiy.  'H  hk  hyriarfo^Sy  kirXovv.  *Api$fihs  ydp 
iarif  Koi  M  fierpwai,  Tb  V  ahrh  odfrioy  kcu  9i6ti 
tA  flip  i.ir6  'njs  fficfipvis  obx  kmlffrpo^  r&  (^  rov 
X^pov  kyrUrrpo^  *0  fiiw  yiip  ^OKpiriis  iyotyurr^s 
[ica2  fiifiriT'^s'']  6  9k  x^P^^t  ^frrov  /ufAurai.  Arist. 
Prob,  xix.  15.  *  Why  are  themes  (nomoi)  not 
used  in  antistrophic  singing,  while  all  other 
choral  singing  is  employed?  Is  it  because 
themes  belong  to  public  performers  who  are 
already  able  to  imitate  and  extend,  and  hence 
would  make  their  song  long  and  very  figu- 
rate?  For  melodies,  like  words,  follow  imi- 
tation and  change.  It  is  more  necessary  for 
melody  to  imitate,  than  for  words.  Where- 
fore dithyrambic  poets  also  when  they  became 
mimetic,  disused  their  previous  antistrophic 
singing.  The  reason  is  that  formerly  gentle- 
men (eleutheroi)  used  to  sing  the  choruses 
themselves.  It  was  difficult  for  many  to  sing 
like  public  performers.   So  they  rather  intoned 


suitable  melodies.  For  it  is  easier  for  one  to 
make  numerons  variations  than  for  many  to 
do  so,  and  for  a  public  performer  than  for 
those  who  retained  old  usage.  Hence  the 
melodies  were  made  simpler  for  them.  Kow 
antistrophic  singing  is  simple,  for  it  depends 
on  number,  and  is  measured  by  a  unit.  The 
same  reason  shews  why  the  parts  of  the  actors 
are  not  antistrophic,  but  those  of  the  chorus 
are  so.  For  the  actor  is  a  pnblic  performer 
[and  a  mime],  but  the  chorus  does  not  imitate 
so  well.' — Translator.] 

*  [Prof.  Helmholtz's  observations  on  speak- 
ing must  be  read  in  reference  to  North  Qerman 
habits  only. — Translator.] 

f  [By  no  means  nniformly,  even  in  North 
Germany.  The  habits  of  different  nations 
here  vary  greatly.  In  Norway  and  in  Sweden 
the  voice  is  regularly  raised  on  unemphatie 
syllables.  In  Scotland  the  emphasis  is  often 
marked  by  lowering  the  ^ich,— Translator.] 


Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


CHAP.    XIII. 


PERIOD  OF  HOMOPHONIC  MUSIC. 


«39 


having  no  accompanying  harmonies.  Nevertheless  our  recitative,  when  well  per- 
formed, will  give  us  a  better  conception  of  the  degree  in  which  the  expression  of 
the  words  can  be  enhanced  by  musical  recitation,  than  we  can  obtain  from  the 
monotonous  repetition  of  the  Roman  liturgy,  although  the  latter  perhaps  is  more 
nearly  related  in  kind  to  ancient  recitation  than  the  former.  The  settlement  of 
the  Roman  liturgy  by  Pope  Gregory  the  Great  (a.d.  590  to  604)  reaches  back  to  a 
time  in  which  reminiscences  of  the  ancient  art,  although  faded  and  deformed, 
might  have  been  in  some  degree  handed  down  by  tradition,  especially  if,  as  we  are 
probably  entitled  to  assume,  Gregory  really  did  little  more  than  finally  establish 
the  Roman  school  of  singing  which  had  existed  from  the  time  of  Pope  Sylvester 
(a.d.  314  to  335).*  The  majority  of  these  formulae  for  lessons,  collects,  &c., 
evidently  imitate  the  cadence  of  ordinary  speech.  They  proceed  at  an  equal 
height ;  particular,  emphatic,  or  non-Latin  words  are  somewhat  altered  in  pitch ; 
&nd  for  the  punctuation  certain  concluding  forms  are  prescribed,  as  the  following  f 
for  lessons,  according  to  the  customs  of  Miinster.f 


if 


Sie    can-ta    com-maj 
Thua  sing  the    com -ma, 


sic    du  -  o  punc'ta: 
and  thus  the    co  -  Ion : 


nc     ve  •  ro  punc-tum, 
and  thus  the  full  stop. 


i 


1^^ 


s 


8ic      sig  -  wu/m     in  -  Ur  -  ro  -  ga   -   ti    -    o  -  nis? 
Thus    sing     the    mark    of        in  -  ter  -  ro    -   ga  -  tion? 


These  and  similar  final  formulae  were  varied  according  to  the  solemnity  of  the 
feast,  the  subject  treated,  the  rank  of  the  priest  that  sang  and  that  answered,  and  % 
Bo  on.f  It  is  easy  to  see  that  they  strove  to  imitate  the  natural  cadences  of 
ordinary  speech,  and  to  give  them  solemnity  by  eliminating  their  individual  irregu- 
larities. Of  course  in  such  fixed  formula  no  regard  can  be  paid  to  the  grammatical 
sense  of  the  clauses,  which  suffers  much  in  various  ways  from  the  intoning. 
Similarly  we  may  suppose  that  the  ancient  tragic  poets  prescribed  the  cadences  of 
speech  to  their  actors,  and  preserved  them  by  a  musical  accompaniment.  And 
since  ancient  tragedy  kept  much  further  aloof  from  immediate  external  realism  than 
our  modem  drama,  as  is  shewn  by  the  artificial  rhythms,  the  unusual  rolling  words, 
the  immovable  strange  masks,  it  could  admit  of  a  more  singing  tone  for  declamation 
than  would,  perhaps,  please  our  modem  ears.  Then  we  must  remember  that  by 
emphasising  or  increasing  the  loudness  of  certain  words,  and  by  rapidity  or  slow- 
ness of  speech,  or  pantomimic  action,  much  life  can  be  thrown  into  delivery  of  this 
kind,  which  would  certainly  be  insufferably  monotonous  if  not  thus  enlivened. 

But  in  any  case  homophonic  music,  even  when  in  olden  time  it  had  to  ac-  ^ 
company  extensive  poems  of  the  highest  character,  necessarily  played  an  utterly 
subordinate  part.  The  musical  turns  must  have  entirely  depended  on  the  changing 
sense  of  the  words,  and  could  have  had  no  independent  artistic  value  or  connection 
without  them.  A  peculiar  melody  for  singing  hexameters  throughout  an  epic,  or 
iambic  trimeters  throughout  a  tragedy,  would  have  been  insupportable.t     Those 


*  [These  are  the  dates  of  his  reign. 
HuHah  says  the  school  was  fonnded  in  a.d. 
350.— Translator.] 

t  Antony,  *  Lehrbuch  des  Gregorianischen 
Kirchengesanges '  [Maniuil  of  Gregorian 
Church-music],  Miinster,  1829.  According  to 
the  inionnation  collected  by  F^tis  (in  his 
JJistoire  gin&rale  de  Mtisique,  Paris,  1869, 
vol.  i.  chap,  vi.),  it  has  become  donbtfal 
whether  this  system  of  declamation  with  pre- 
scribed cadences,  is  not  rather  to  be  deduced 


from  the  Jewish  ritual  chants.  In  the  oldest 
manuscripts  of  the  Old  Testament,  there  are 
25  different  signs  employed  to  denote  cadences 
and  melodic  phrases  of  this  kind.  The  fact 
that  the  corresponding  signs  of  the  Greek 
Church  are  Egyptian  demotic  characters,  hints 
at  a  still  older  Egyptian  origin  for  this  nota> 
tion. 

X  [We  must  remember  that  the  Greek  and 
Latin  so-called  accents  consisted  solely  in 
alterations  of  pitch,  and  hence  to  a  certain 


Digitized  by  V^OOQIC 


240  PEEIOD  OP  HOMOPHONIC  MUSIC.  part  m. 

melodies  (vofu>i)  which  were  allotted  to  odes  and  tragic  ohorases,  were  certainly 
freer  and  more  independent.  For  odes  there  were  also  well-known  melodies  (the 
names  of  some  of  them  are  preserved)  to  which  fresh  poems  were  continually 
composed. 

In  the  great  artistic  works  just  mentioned,  then,  music  must  have  been  entirely 
subordinate ;  independently,  it  could  only  have  formed  sliort  pieces.  Now  this  is 
closely  connected  with  the  development  of  homophonio  music  as  a  musical  system. 
Among  the  nations  who  possess  such  music  we  always  find  certain  degrees  of  pitch 
selected  for  the  melodies  to  move  in.  These  scales  are  very  various  in  kind,  partly, 
it  would  seem,  very  arbitrary,  so  that  many  appear  to  us  quite  strange  and  incom- 
prehensible, and  yet  the  best  gifted  among  those  nations  which  possess  them,  as 
the  Greeks,  Arabs,  and  Indians,  have  developed  them  in  an  extremely  subtile  and 
varied  manner.     [See  App.  XX.  sect.  K.] 

f  When  speaking  of  these  systems  of  tones,  it  becomes  a  question  of  essential 
importance  for  our  present  purpose,  to  inquire  whether  they  are  based  upon  any 
determinate  reference  of  aU  the  tones  in  the  scale  to  one  single  principal  and 
fundamental  tone,  the  tonic  or  key-note.  Modem  music  effects  a  purely  musical 
internal  connection  among  all  the  tones  in  a  composition,  by  making  their  rela- 
tionship to  one  tone  as  perceptible  as  possible  to  the  ear.  Tliis  predominance 
of  the  tonic,  as  the  link  which  connects  all  the  tones  of  a  piece,  we  may,  with 
F^tis,  term  the  principle  of  tonality.  This  learned  musician  has  properly  drawn 
attention  to  the  fact  that  tonality  is  developed  in  very  different  degrees  and 
manners  in  the  melodies  of  different  nations.  Thus  in  the  songs  of  the  modem 
Greeks,  and  chants  of  the  Greek  Church,  and  the  Gregorian  tones  of  the  Koman 
Church,  they  are  not  developed  in  a  manner  which  is  easy  to  harmonise,  whereas, 
according  to  F^tis,*  it  is  on  the  whole  easy  to  add  accompanying  harmonies 
to  the  old  melodies  of  the  northern  nations  of  German,  Celtic,  and  Sclavonic 

^  origin. 

It  is  indeed  remarkable  that  though  the  musical  writings  of  the  Greeks  often 
treat  subtile  points  at  great  length,  and  give  the  most  exact  information  about  all 
other  peculiarities  of  the  scale,  they  say  nothing  intelligible  about  a  relation  which 
in  our  modem  system  stands  first  of  all,  and  always  makes  itself  most  disthietly 
sensible.  The  only  hints  to  be  found  conceming  the  existence  of  the  tonic  are 
not  in  especial  musical  writings,  but  as  before  in  the  works  of  Aristotle,  who 
asks : — 

*  Why  is  it  that  if  any  one  alters  the  tone  on  the  middle  string  (fiiayi)  after  the 
others  have  been  tuned,  and  plays,  every  thing  sounds  amiss,  not  merely  when  he 
comes  to  this  middle  tone,  but  throughout  the  whole  melody  ?  but  if  he  alters  the 
tone  played  by  the  forefinger  t  or  any  other,  the  difference  is  only  perceived  when 
that  string  is  struck  ?  Is  there  a  good  reason  for  this  ?  All  good  melodies  often 
employ  the  tone  of  the  middle  string,  and  good  composers  often  come  upon  it, 

%  and  if  they  leave  it  recur  to  it  again  ;  but  this  is  not  the  case  with  any  other 
tone.'  Then  he  compares  the  tone  of  the  middle  string  with  conjunctions  in 
language,  such  as  '  and '  [and  '  then '],  without  which  language  could  not  exist,  and 
proceeds  to  say :  *  In  this  way  the  tone  of  the  middle  string  is  a  link  between 
tones,  especially  of  the  best  tones,  because  its  tone  most  frequently  recurs.' t 

extent  determined  a  melody.    See  Dionysias  joined  with  a  constant  quantity  or  rhythm. — 

of    HalicamassuSt    ircpi    trwddatws    dvofidruvt  Translator.] 

chap,  xi.,  where  we  also  find  that  in  his  day  *  Fetis,  Biographie  univerBelle  des  Muti- 

(first  century  before  Christ)  the  musical  com-  dens^  vol.  i.  p.  126. 

posers  transgressed  at  pleasure  the  rules  of  f  [The  forefinger  is  t  Kixaofis^  the  note 

both  accent  and  quantity.    But  if  the  written  played  by  it  is  ^  Kix^ofoi,  accent  and  gender 

accents  in  Greek,  and  the  accents  as  deter-  both  differing. --Tmn«2ator.] 

mined  by  the  rules  of  grammarians  in  Latin,  1  Ai^  rl,  Am  fi4y  r»  rV  m^^*'  «rir^  4fU»r. 

are  carefully  examined,  it  will  be  found  that  apfiitras   [82]   r&f    &\Xas  x^P^^i  K^XPVai  r^ 

every  line  in  a  Greek  or  Latin  poem  had  its  hpydvt^^  ov  fi6trov  Urop  Karh  rhr  r^f  fi^aris  yitn^ 

own  diRtinct  melody,  the  art  of  the  poet  being  rcu   ^^yyoy^   Ai/irc?,   «ral    ^ytrat    Mt^fMvrop, 

shewn  by  the  great  variability  of  pitch  con-  oAAa  kuI  xarii  rhy  iXXtfy  fitKq^iay  -  imp  £*  riiw 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


CHAP.  xin. 


PERIOD  OF  HOMOPHONIC  MUSIC. 


241 


And  in  another  place  we  find  the  same  question  with  a  slightly  different  answer, 
Why  do  the  other  tones  sound  badly  when  the  tone  of  the  middle  string  is 
altered  ?  bat  if  the  tone  of  the  middle  string  remains,  and  one  of  the  others  is 
altered,  the  altered  one  alone  is  spoiled  ?  Is  it  because  that  all  are  tuned  and  have 
a  certain  relation  to  the  tone  of  the  middle  string,  and  the  order  of  each  is  deter- 
mined by  that  ?  The  reason  of  the  tuning  and  connection  being  removed,  then, 
things  no  longer  appear  the  same.'*  In  these  sentences  the  esthetic  significance  of 
the  tonic,  under  the  name  of  *  the  tone  of  the  middle  string,'  is  very  accurately 
described.  To  this  we  may  add  that  the  Pythagoreans  compared  the  tone  of  the 
middle  string  with  the  sun,  and  the  other  tones  in  the  scale  with  the  planets.f  It 
appears  as  if  it  had  been  usual  to  begin  with  the  tone  of  the  middle  string  above 
mentioned,  for  we  read  in  the  33rd  problem  of  Aristotle :  *  Why  is  it  more  agree- 
able to  proceed  from  high  pitch  to  low  pitch,  than  from  low  pitch  to  high  pitch  ?  Can 
it  be  that  we  thus  begin  at  the  beginning  ?  for  the  tone  of  the  middle  string  is  also  ^ 
the  leader  of  the  tetrachord  and  highest  in  pitch.  The  second  way  would  be  to  begin 
at  the  end  instead  of  at  the  beginning.  Or  can  it  be  that  tones  of  lower  pitch 
sound  nobler  and  more  euphonious  after  tones  of  high  pitch  ?  '  j:  This  seems  also  to 
shew  that  it  was  not  the  custom  to  end  with  the  tone  of  the  middle  string,  which 
commenced,  but  with  the  tone  of  lowest  pitch  [produced  by  the  uppermost  string 
or  Hypate],  of  which  last  tone  Aristotle,  in  his  4th  problem,  says  that,  as  opposed 
to  its  neighbour,  the  tone  of  lowest  pitch  but  one,  [due  to  the  string  of  highest 
position  but  one,  or  Parhypatit]  it  is  sung  with  complete  relaxation  of  all  the  effort 
that  is  felt  in  the  other.§  These  words  of  Aristotle  may  certainly  be  applied  to 
the  national  Doric  scale  of  the  Greeks,  which,  increased  by  Pythagoras  to  eight 
tones,  was  as  follows : — 


Huupiptuf  lUvoVj  Urcof  K&Kcdqi  r\s  XP^'"'^ :  *H 
€v\Ayns  rovro  avfjificdyu ;  wdvra  ykp  rk  xf>^<rr& 

iyoBol  tronrraL,  wvicriL  xphs  riiv  fA^trnv  kwamStvi  * 
jc^  harMwfft  tox^  hrav^pxovrcu  •  wphs  8i  &AAt}K 
O0TW9  ovZffilay.  KaBdvep  iK  rwv  \6yutr  itrlour 
i^atptBirrwv  awh^iryLfav^  ovk  tfrriv  6  \Syos  *Z?<Afiv- 
ik6s  '  (oTov  rh  ri,  iced  rh  rol)  Koi  Ivioi  8i  ovOkv 
Xuwovci  *  8(jt  rh  rots  fi^v,  kyayKcuov  ttvvu  xp^^M 
voAA<£iccf ,  ^  OVK  (ffrai  \6yos  *E?i\rjvuc6s '  rols  8i, 
ft^*  o5r«  KoL  r&y  ^B6rfyiav  ^  inivrt^  &<nr€p  ff^l^ir- 
Ia6$  4art,  koI  fidXtirra  rwv  KaXAv,  htk  rh  wXturriKis 
iyvwdpx*iy  "rhy  ^^yyoy  a^ijs.  Arist.  Prob.  zix. 
20.  This  passage  has  also  been  partly  quoted 
by  Ambrosch.  [The  names  of  Greek  tones 
were  those  of  the  strings  on  the  lyre  by  which 
they  were  played,  jast  as  if  in  English  we  were 
to  call  the  tones  g,  d\  a',  e'\  the  tones  of  the 
fourth,  third,  second,  and  first  strings  resi>eo- 
tively,  because  they  are  produced  as  the  open 
notes  of  these  strings  on  the  violin,  and  con- 
tracted them  to  fourth^  thirds  &c.,  only  omit- 
ting the  word  string.  As  the  violin  when  held 
sideways  in  playing,  throws  the  g  string  upper- 
most, and  the  e^'  string  the  lowermost,  we 
might  in  the  same  way  call  g  the  *  up}>ermost 
note,'  virch-i),  although  lowest  in  pit(di,  and  e" 
the  *  lowermost  note,*  rfrr%  although  highest 
in  pitch.  Then  d  might  be  called  the  middle^ 
fidafi,  being  really  the  key-note  of  the  violin 
and  one  of  the  two  middle  strings.  This  illus- 
trates the  Greek  names  very  closely,  for  the 
lyre  was  held  with  the  string  sounding  the 
lowest  note,  uppermost.  See  tiie  scale  on  the 
next  page. — Translator,] 

*  Aik  rl,  iiiy  fi^v  ii  fiitnj  KurnOri,  Koi  al  &AAai 
X«pSci2  ^x^'*^^*  ^€yy6/i€vai'  (one  of  my  col- 
leagues. Prof.  Stark,  conjectures  that  in  place 
of    ^€yy6fitycu,  which    makes   nonsense,   we 


should  read  ^fip6/iweu  *)  ii»  82  aZ  4i  fih^  uAvjf, 
r&y  8*  dKXmy  rls  KuniO^,  Konfiuva  fi6y7i  <l>64y-  «r 
yerai ;  (for  which  Prof.  Stark  again  proposes 
^B^lpercu  ;)  *H  8ti  t^  lipfiScBcu  i<rrly  iiiriffais,  [rh 
82  Ix**"  *■»*  ^P^f  '^y  /jJiniy  kwdtreus],  koL  ^  rd^ts 
if  iKoffrriSf  ff8i|  8t'  iKtlyriy  ;  itpBiyros  olyrov  ahlov 
rod  7ipfi6<rBat  koL  rod  (rvytxoyros,  oIk  (ri  6fU}lo05 
^yercu  irdpx^uf-    Arist.  Prob.  xix.  36. 

f  Nicomachns,  Harmonidt  lib.  i.  p.  6, 
ed.  Meibomii.  [The  following  is  Nioomachus'a 
arrangement  of  the  comparison,  with  his 
reasons:   . 

Saturn  hypaU,  as  being  highest  in  position, 
traroy  ybip  rh  kytSrraeroy. 

Jupiter  ]^rhypatli  as  next  highest  to 
Saturn. 

Mars  lichanos  or  hypermesi^  as  between 
Jupiter  and  the  Sun. 

The  Sun  mesBt  as  lying  in  the  middle,  the 
fourth  from  either  end,  middlemost  string  and 
planet.  «r 

Mercury  paramese^  as  lying  between  the  ^' 
Sun  and  Venus. 

Venus  paraneatBt  as  lying  just  above  the 
moon. 

The  Moon  neate,  as  being  lowest  of  all  in 
position  and  next  the  earth,  iced  yitp  yiwov^  rh 
Kardararoy, — Translator,] 

\  Lib.  ri  €vapfAOffr6rtpoy  iwh  rov  &^4ot  M  rh 
fiapv,  fj  iiTh  rov  fiapdos  M  rh  o^v;  Tl6r€pov  St< 
rh  emh  rijs  if>x^*  yiyerai  Apx^^Bcu;  ^  ykp  fjiiari 
K<d  riy€fiiiy  o^vrdrri  rov  rtrpax^pZov.  Th  82  ovk 
iir*  &pxv^t  ^^'  A*"^  rtXtvrrjs.  *H  Jrt  r6  fiapb 
iLw6  rov  6^4o9  y^yyai&r^poy,  koJl  th^y^^poy ; 
Arist.  Prob,  xix.  33. 

§  Lih  rl  82  ra{miy  [r^y  irapvwdniy]  xa^frrcis 
[fSovcri],  riiy  82  lirrdrriy  p^Ziws '  KcUroi  SUfftf 
4Kar4pas ;  ^  Zri  fi^r*  dy4atus  4i  irdrri,  ica2  &fxa 
fierk  r^y  avffroffiy  i\a^p6y  rh  &yv  fidWfiv ; 
Arist.  Prob.  xix.  4. 

Digitized  by  V^jOQQIC 


242  PEEIOD  OF  HOMOPHONIC  MUSIC.  part  m. 


Tetrachord   of 
lowest  pitch 


Tetrachord    of 
highest  pitch 


'E  HypSte [^drii       nppermost  string] 

F  Parhypate        ....  [irapvwdrri  next  to  nppermost  string] 

G  Lichfinos         ....  [A^x^''^''     forefinger  string] 

.A  Mese  (tone  of  middle  string)   .  {/i4(ni         middle  string] 

B  ParamSse        ....  [wapafU<rri  next  to  middle  string] 

C  Trite [rplrn         third  string] 

D  Paranete [waparfrrv  next  to  lowermost  string] 

E  Nete [1^17         lowermost  string] 


In  modem  phraseology  the  last  description  cited  from  Aristotle  implies  that  the 
Parhypate  was  a  kind  of  descending  '  leading  note  '  to  the  Hjpatd.  In  the  leading 
tone  there  is  perceptible  effort,  which  ceases  on  its  falling  into  the  fundamental 
tone. 

If,  then,  the  tone  of  the  middle  string  answers  to  the  tonic,  the  Hypate,  which 

^  is  its  Fifth,  will  answer  to  the  dominant.  For  our  modem  feeling  it  is  far  more 
necessary  to  close  with  the  tonic  than  to  begin  with  it,  and  hence  we  usually  take 
the  final  tone  of  a  piece  to  be  its  tonic  without  farther  inquiry.  Modem  music, 
however,  usually  introduces  the  tonic  also  in  the  first  beat  of  the  opening  bar. 
The  whole  mass  of  tone  is  developed  firom  the  tonic  and  returns  into  it.  Modem 
musicians  cannot  obtain  complete  repose  at  the  end  unless  the  series  of  tones  con- 
verges into  its  connecting  centre. 

Ancient  Greek  music  seems,  then,  to  have  deviated  firom  ours  by  ending  on  the 
dominant  instead  of  the  tonic.  And  this  is  in  full  agreement  with  the  intonation 
of  speech.  We  have  seen  that  the  end  of  an  affirmative  sentence  is  Ukewise 
formed  on  the  Fifth  next  below  the  principal  tone.*  This  peculiarity  has  also 
been  generally  preserved  in  modem  recitative,  in  which  the  singer  usuaUy  ends  on 
the  dominant;  the  accompanying  instruments  then  make  this  tone  part  of  the 
chord  of  the  dominant  Seventh,  leading  to  the  tonic  chord,  and  thus  make  a  close 

^  on  the  tonic  in  accordance  with  our  present  musical  feeling.  Now  since  Greek 
music  was  cultivated  by  the  recitation  of  epic  hexameters  and  iambic  trimeters, 
we  should  not  be  surprised  if  the  above-mentioned  peculiarities  of  chanting  were 
so  predominant  in  the  melodies  of  odes  that  Aristotle  could  regard  them  as  the 

rule.t 

From  the  facts  just  adduced  it  follows  (and  this  is  what  we  are  chiefly  con- 
cerned with)  that  the  Greeks,  among  whom  our  diatonic  scale  first  arose,  were  not 
without  a  certain  esthetic  feeling  for  tonality,  but  that  they  had  not  developed  it  so 
decisively  as  in  modem  music.  Indeed,  it  does  not  appear  to  have  even  entered 
into  the  technical  rules  for  constructing  melodies.  Hence  Aristotle,  who  treated 
music  esthetically,  is  the  only  known  writer  who  mentions  it;  musical  writers 
proper  do  not  speak  of  it  at  all.  And  unfortunately  the  indications  famished  by 
Aristotle  are  so  meagre,  that  doubt  enough  still  exists.  For  example,  he  says 
nothing  about  the  differences  of  the  various  musical  modes  in  reference  to  their 
m  principal  tone,  so  that  the  most  important  point  of  all  from  which  we  should  wish 
to  regard  the  construction  of  the  musical  scale,  is  almost  entirely  obscured. 

The  reference  to  a  tonic  is  more  distinctly  made  out  in  the  scales  of  the  old 
Christian  ecclesiastical  music.  Originally  the  four  so-called  authentic  scales  were 
distinguished,  as  they  had  been  laid  down  by  Ambrose  of  Milan  (elected  Bishop 
A.D.  374,  died  A.D.  398).  Not  one  of  these  agrees  with  any  one  of  our  scales.  The 
four  plagal  scales  afterwards  added  by  Gregory,  are  no  scales  at  all  in  our  sense  of 
the  word.    The  four  authentic  scales  of  Ambrose  t  are : 

♦  [This  wonld  be  entirely  crossed  by  the  ment  of  the  Homeric  Ode  to  Demeter,  which 

ancient  Greek  system  of  pitch-accents,  just  as  has  been  published  by  B.  Marcello,  shews  the 

it  now  is  by  a  similar  system  in  Norwegian,  above-mentioned  peculiarity  very  distinctly, 
where  the  pitch  may  rise  for  60^^  affirmative  X   C^^*  Bockstro  in  his  article  '  Ambro- 

and  interrogative  sentences.     See  p.  239^'-,  sian  Chant,'  in  Grove's  Dictionary  of  Music^ 

note  %,— Translator.']  ^  ^  states  that  this  attribution  of  four  authentic 

f  Among  the  presumed  ancient  melodies  scales  to  St.  Ambrose  has  not  been  proved. — 

which  have  been  handed  down  to  us,  the  frag-  Translator.] 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


CHAP.  XIII.  PERIOD  OF  HOMOPHONIC  MUSIC.  243 

i)DEFGABcd 
2)  E  F  G  A  B  c  d  e 
l)  F  G  A  B  c  d  e  f 
/^)     G    A    B     c     d     e    f    g 

Perhaps,  however,  the  change  of  B  into  B^  was  allowed  from  the  first,  and 
this  would  make  the  first  scale  agree  with  our  descending  scale  of  D  minor,  and  the 
third  scale  would  become  our  scale  of  F  major.  The  old  rule  was  that  songs  in 
the  first  scale  should  end  in  2),  those  in  the  second  in  E,  those  in  the  third  in  F, 
and  those  in  the  fourth  in  G,  This  marked  out  these  tones  as  tonics  in  our  sense 
of  the  word.  But  the  rule  was  not  strictly  observed.  The  conclusion  might  fall 
on  other  tones  of  the  scale,  the  so-called  confinal  tones,  and  at  last  the  confusion 
became  so  great  that  no  one  was  able  to  say  exactly  how  the  scale  was  to  be 
recognised,  all  kinds  of  insufficient  rules  were  formulated,  and  at  last  musicians 
clung  to  the  mechanical  expedient  of  fixing  upon  certain  initial  and  concluding  ^ 
phrases,  called  tropes^  as  characterising  the  scale. 

Hence  although  the  rule  of  tonahty  had  been  already  remarked  in  these 
medieval  ecclesiastical  scales,  the  rule  was  so  unsettled  and  admitted  so  many 
exceptions,  that  the  feeling  of  tonahty  must  have  been  much  less  developed  than 
in  modem  music. 

The  Indians  also  hit  upon  the  conception  of  a  tonic,  although  their  music  is 
likewise  unisonal.  They  called  the  tonic  AnsaJ*  Indian  melodies  as  transcribed 
by  Enghsh  travellers,  seem  to  be  very  like  modem  European  melodies.f  F6tis 
and  Goussemaker  j:  have  made  the  same  remark  respecting  the  few  known  remains 
of  old  German  and  Geltic  melodies. 

Although,  therefore,  homophonic  music  was  [possibly]  not  entirely  without  a  refe- 
rence to  some  tonic,  or  predominant  tone,  such  a  tone  was  beyond  cJl  dispute  much 
more  weakly  developed  than  in  modem  music,  where  a  few  consecutive  chords 
suffice  to  establish  the  scale  in  which  that  portion  of  the  piece  is  written.  The  f 
cause  of  this  seems  to  me  traceable  to  the  undeveloped  condition  and  subordinate 
part  which  characterises  homophonic  music.  Melodies  which  move  up  and  down 
in  a  few  tones  which  are  easily  comprehended,  and  are  connected,  not  by  some 
musical  contrivance,  but  by  the  words  of  a  poem,  do  not  require  the  consistent 
application  of  any  contrivance,  to  combine  them.  Even  in  modem  recitative 
tonality  is  much  less  firmly  established  than  in  other  forms  of  composition.  The 
necessity  for  a  steady  connection  of  masses  of  tone  by  purely  musical  relations, 
does  not  dawn  distinctly  on  our  feehng,  until  we  have  to  form  into  one  artistic 
whole  large  masses  of  tone,  which  have  their  own  independent  significance  without 
the  cement  of  poetry. 

♦  Jones,  On  the  Music  0/  the  IndianSt  "  By  the  word  vddi,"  aays  the  commentator, 

translated  by  Dalberg,  pp.  36,  37.     [Sir  W.  **  he  means  the  note,  which  announces  and 

Joneses  tract,  with  many  others,  is  reprinted  ascertains  the  B&ga  [tane],  and  which  may  be 

in  Sonrindro  Mohnn  T^gore's  Hindu  Music  considered  as  the  parent  and  origin  of  the 

from  various  Authors,    This  is  what  he  says  graha  and  nydsa,"   This  clearly  shews,  I  think  IT 

of    the  ans'a,  p.   149  of  Tagore :    *  Since  it  [says  Sir  W.  Jones],  that  the  ans'a  must  be 

appears  from  the  N&^yan  [a  Sanscrit  treatise  the  tonic ;  and  we  shall  find  that  the  two 

on  music],  that  36  modes  are  in  general  use,  other  notes  are  generally  its  third  and  fifth, 

and  the  rest  very  rarely  applied  to  practice,  I  or  the  mediant  and  dominant.    In  the  poem 

shall  exhibit  only  the  scales  of  the  6  B&gas  entitled  Mdgha  there  is    a  musical    simile, 

[tunes]  and  30  B^iginis  [female  personification  which  may  illustrate  and  confirm  our  idea. 

of  tunes  in  Hindu  music]  according  to  S6ma.  [I  give  the  translation  only.]      "  From  the 

.  .  .  Three  distinguished  sounds  in  each  mode  greatness,  from  the  transcendent  quaUties  of 

[as  Sir  W.Jones  translates  r^]  are  called  ffra/ia,  that  hero,  eager  for  conquest,  other   kings 

nydsa,  ans'a,  and  the  writer  of  the  Nar&yan  march  in  subordination  to  him,  as  other  notes 

defines  them  in  the  two  following  couplets.  are  subordinate  to  the  ans'a. '" — Translator  J] 
[I  give  the  translation  only.]    **  The  note  called  f  [The   construction   and  time    are  very 

graha  is  placed  at  the  beginning,  and  that  different.    The  scales  are  extremely  variable. 

named  nydsa  at  the  end,  of  a  song ;  that  note.  The  results  are  very  imi>erfectly  represented 

which  displays  the  peculiar  melody,  and  to  by  our  present  musical  notation. — Translator.] 
which  all  the  others  are  subordinate,  that,  ^  Histoire  de  VHarmonie  au  moyen  Age, 

which  is  always  of  the  greatest  use,  is  like  a  Pans,  1852,  pp.  5-7. 
sovereign,  though  a  mere  aTw'a  or  portion." 


Digitized  by 


'-^QQgle 


244  PERIOD  OF  POLYPHONIC  MUSIC.  part  m. 

2.  Polyphonic  Music. 

The  second  stage  of  musical  development  is  the  polyphonic  music  of  the  middle 
ages.  It  is  usual  to  cite  as  the  first  invented  part-music,  the  so-called  organum  or 
diaphony,  as  originally  described  by  the  Flemish  monk  Hucbald  at  the  beginning 
of  the  tenth  century.  In  this,  two  voices  are  said  to  have  proceeded  in  Fifths  or 
Fourths,  with  occasional  doublings  of  one  or  both  in  Octaves.  This  would  pro- 
duce intolerable  music  for  modem  ears.  But  according  to  0.  Paul*  the  meaning 
is  not  that  the  two  voices  sang  at  the  same  time,  but  that  there  was  a  respon- 
sive repetition  of  a  melody  in  a  transposed  condition,  in  which  case  Hucbald  would 
have  been  the  inventor  of  a  principle  which  subsequently  became  so  important  in 
the  fugue  and  sonata. 

The  first  undoubted  form  of  part-music  intentionally  for  several  voices,  was  the 

^  so-called  discanPus,  which  became  known  at  the  end  of  the  eleventh  century  in 
France  and  Flanders.  The  oldest  specimens  of  this  kind  of  music  which  have 
been  preserved  are  of  the  following  description.  Two  entirely  different  melodies 
— and  to  all  appearance  the  more  different  the  better — were  adapted  to  one  another 
by  shght  changes  in  rhythm  or  pitch,  until  they  formed  a  tolerably  consonant  whole. 
At  first,  indeed,  there  seems  to  have  been  an  inclination  for  coupling  a  liturgical 
formula  with  a  rather  *  slippery '  song.  The  first  of  such  examples  could  scarcely 
have  been  intended  for  more  than  musical  tricks  to  amuse  social  meetings.  It  was 
a  new  and  amusing  discovery  that  two  totally  independent  melodies  might  be  sung 
together  and  yet  sound  well. 

The  principle  of  discant  was  fertile,  and  its  nature  was  suitable  for  develop- 
ment at  that  period.  Polyphonic  music  proper  was  its  issue.  Different  voices, 
each  proceeding  independently  and  singing  its  own  melody,  had  to  be  united  in 
such  a  way  as  to  produce  either  no  dissonances,  or  merely  transient  ones  which 

f  were  readily  resolved.  Consonance  was  not  the  object  in  view,  but  its  opposite, 
dissonance,  was  to  be  avoided.  All  interest  was  concentrated  on  the  motion  of  the 
voices.  To  keep  the  various  parts  together,  time  had  to  be  strictly  observed,  and 
hence  the  influence  of  discant  developed  a  system  of  musical  rhythm,  which  again 
contributed  to  infase  greater  power  and  importance  into  melodic  progression. 
There  was  no  division  of  time  in  the  Gregorian  Canttis  firmus.  The  rhythm  of 
dance  music  was  probably  extremely  simple.  Moreover,  melodic  movement  in- 
creased in  richness  and  interest  as  the  parts  were  multiphed.  But  the  establish- 
ment of  an  artistic  connection  between  the  different  voices,  which,  as  we  have  seen, 
were  at  first  perfectly  free,  required  a  new  invention,  and  this,  though  it  cropped 
up  at  first  in  a  very  humble  form,  has  ended  by  obtaining  predominant  importance 
in  the  whole  art  of  modem  musical  composition.  This  invention  consisted  in 
causing  a  musical  phrase  which  had  been  sung  by  one  voice  to  be  repeated  by 
another.    Thus  arose  canonic  imitation,  which  may  be  met  with  sporadically  as 

If  early  as  in  the  twelfth  century .f  This  subsequently  developed  into  a  higlily 
artistic  system,  especially  among  Netherland  composers,  who,  it  must  be  owned, 
ended  by  often  shewing  more  calculation  than  taste  in  their  compositions. 

But  by  this  kind  of  polyphonic  music— the  repetition  of  the  same  melodic 
phrases  in  succession  by  different  voices — it  first  became  possible  to  compose 
musical  pieces  on  an  extensive  plan,  owing  their  connection  not  to  any  union  with 
another  fine  art — poetry,  but  to  purely  musical  contrivances.  This  kind  of  music 
also  was  especially  suited  to  ecclesiastical  songs,  in  which  the  chorus  had  to  express 
the  feelings  of  a  whole  congregation  of  worshippers,  each  with  his  own  peculiar 
disposition.  It  was,  however,  not  confined  to  ecclesiastical  compositions,  but  was 
also  applied  to  secular  songs  (madrigals).  The  sole  form  of  harmonic  music  yet 
known,  which  could  be  adapted  to  artistic  cultivation,  was  that  founded  on  canonic 

*  Oeschichte  des  Claviers  [History  of  the      nos,  PI.  zxvii.  No.  iy.,  translated  \n  p.  xxviL 
Pianoforte],  Leipzig,  i868,  p.  49.  No.  xxix. 

f  Coussemaker,  loc.  cii-    .Discant :  Custodi 

Digitized  by  VjOOQIC 


CHAP.  XIII.  PEBIOD  OP  POLYPHONIC  MUSIC.  245 

repetitions.  If  this  had  been  rejected,  nothing  but  homophonio  music  remained. 
Hence  we  find  a  number  of  songs  set  ad  strict  canons  or  with  canonical  repetitions, 
although  they  were  entirely  unsuited  for  such  a  heavy  form  of  composition.  Even  the 
oldest  examples  of  instrumental  compositions  in  several  parts,  the  dance  music  of 
1529,*  are  written  in  the  form  of  madrigals  and  motets,  a  character  of  composition 
which,  more  freely  treated,  lasted  down  to  the  suites  of  S.  Bach  and  Handel's 
times.  Even  in  the  first  attempts  at  musical  dramas  in  the  sixteenth  century, 
there  was  no  other  way  of  making  the  personages  express  their  feelings  musically, 
than  by  causing  a  chorus  behind  or  upon  the  stage  to  sing  over  some  madrigals  in 
the  fugue  style.  It  is  scarcely  possible  for  us,  from  our  present  point  of  view,  to 
conceive  the  condition  of  an  luH;  which  was  able  to  build  up  the  most  complicated 
constructions  of  voice  parts  in  chorus,  and  was  yet  incapable  of  adding  a  simple 
accompaniment  to  the  melody  of  a  song  or  a  duet,  for  the  purpose  of  filling  up 
the  harmony.  And  yet  when  we  read  how  Giacomo  Peri's  invention  of  recitative  f 
with  a  simple  accompaniment  of  chorus  was  applauded  and  admired  and  what 
contentions  arose  as  to  the  renown  of  the  invention ;  what  attention  Viadana 
excited  when  he  invented  the  addition  of  a  Basso  continuo  for  songs  in  one  or 
two  parts,  as  a  dependent  part  serving  only  to  fill  up  the  harmony  f ;  it  is  impos- 
sible to  doubt  that  this  art  of  accompanying  a  melody  by  chords  (as  any  amateur 
can  now  do  in  the  simplest  maimer  possible)  was  completely  unknown  to  musicians 
up  to  the  end  of  the  sixteenth  century.  It  was  not  till  the  sixteenth  century 
that  composers  became  aware  of  the  meaning  possessed  by  chords  as  forming  an 
harmonic  tissue  independently  of  the  progression  of  parts. 

To  this  condition  of  the  art  corresponded  the  condition  of  the  tonal  system. 
The  old  ecclesiastical  scales  were  retained  in  their  essentials,  the  first  from  D  io  d, 
the  second  from  E  to  6,  the  third  from  Fiof,  and  the  fourth  from  Gix)g.  Of  these 
the  scale  from  F  \of  was  useless  for  harmonic  purposes,  because  it  contained  tlie 
Tritone  F—B,  in  place  of  the  Fourth  F—B^.  Again,  there  was  no  reason  for  ^ 
excluding  the  scales  from  0  to  c  and  AU>  a.  And  thus  the  ecclesiastical  scales 
altered  under  the  infiuence  of  polyphonic  music.  But  as  the  old  unsuitable  names 
were  retained  notwithstanding  the  (Ganges,  there  arose  a  terrible  confusion  in  the 
meaning  attached  to  modes.  It  was  not  till  nearly  the  end  of  this  period  that 
a  learned  theoretician,  Glarean,  undertook  in  his  Dodecachordon  (Basle  1547)  to 
put  some  order  into  the  theory  of  modes.  He  distinguished  twelve  of  them,  six 
authentic  and  six  plagal,  and  assigned  them  Qreek  names,  which  were,  however, 
incorrectly  transferred.  However,  his  nomenclature  for  ecclesiastical  modes  has 
been  generally  followed  ever  since.  The  following  are  Glarean*s  six  authentic  eccle- 
siastical modes,  keys,  or  scales,  with  the  incorrect  Greek  names  he  assigned  to 
them. 

Ionic  .        .        .    CDEFGABc 


Doric  . 
Phrygian 
Lydian 
Mixolydian 
EoUc  • 


DEFOABcd 
EFOABcde 
F  G  A  B  c  d  e  f 
GABodefg 
A  B  c  d  e  f   g  a 


Ionic  answers  to  our  major.  Folic  to  our  minor  system.  Lydian  was  scarcely 
ever  used  in  polyphonic  music  owing  to  the  false  Fourth  F^B,  and  when  it  was 
employed  it  was  altered  in  many  different  ways. 

Inability  to  judge  of  the  musical  significance  of  a  connected  tissue  of  harmonies 
again  appears  in  the  theory  of  the  keys,  by  the  rule,  that  the  key  of  a  polyphonic 
composition  was  determined  by  considering  the  separate  voices  independently. 
Glarean  in  certain  compositions  attributes  different  keys  to  the  tenor  and  bass,  the 
soprano  and  alto«  Zarlino  assumes  the  tenor  as  the  chief  part  for  determining  the 
key. 

*  Winterfeld,  Johannes  Oabrieli  und  sein  Zeitalter,  vol.  ii.  p.  41. 
t  Winterfeld,  ibid,t  vol.  ii.  p.  19  and  p.  59. 

Digitized  by  VjOOQIC 


246  PERIOD  OF  HARMONIC  MUSIC.  pabt  m. 

The  practical  consequences  of  this  neglect  of  harmony  are  conspicuous  in 
various  ways  in  musical  compositions.  The  composers  confined  themselves  on  ihe 
whole  to  the  diatonic  scale ;  '  accidentals/  or  signs  of  alterations  of  tone,  were  seldom 
used.  The  Greeks  had  introduced  the  depression  of  the  tone  Bio  JB\}inek  peculiar 
tetrachord,  that  of  the  synimmenoit  and  this  was  retained.  Besides  this/jf,  c  jf 
and  g  ^  were  used,  to  introduce  leading  tones  in  the  cadences.  Hence  modulation, 
as  we  understand  it,  from  the  key  of  one  tonic  to  that  of  another  with  a  different 
signature  was  almost  entirely  absent.  Moreover,  the  chords  used  by  preference 
down  to  the  end  of  the  fifteenth  century,  were  formed  of  the  Octave  and  Fifth 
without  the  Third,  and  such  chords  now  sound  poor  and  are  avoided  as  much  as 
possible.  To  medieval  composers  who  only  felt  the  want  of  the  most  perfect  con- 
sonances, these  chords  appeared  the  most  agreeable,  and  none  others  might  be  used 
at  the  close  of  a  piece.  The  dissonances  which  occur  are  universally  those  which 
^  arise  from  suspended  and  passing  tones ;  chords  of  the  dominant  Seventh,  which, 
in  modem  harmony,  play  such  an  important  part  in  marking  the  key,  and  in  con- 
necting and  facihtating  progressions,  were  quite  unknown. 

Great,  then,  as  was  the  artistic  advance  in  rhythm  and  the  progression  of  parts, 
during  this  period,  it  did  Httle  more  for  harmony  and  the  tonal  system  than  to 
accumulate  an  imdigested  mass  of  experiments.  Since  the  involved  progression  of 
the  parts  gave  rise  to  chords  in  extremely  varied  transpositions  and  sequences,  the 
musicians  of  this  period  could  not  but  hear  these  chords  and  become  acquainted 
with  their  effects,  however  httle  skill  they  shewed  in  making  use  of  them.  At  any 
rate,  the  experience  of  this  period  prepared  the  way  for  harmonic  music  proper, 
and  made  it  possible  for  musicians  to  produce  it,  when  external  circumstances  forced 
on  the  discovery. 

3.  Habmonig  Music. 

IF  Modem  harmonic  music  is  characterised  by  the  independent  significance  of  its 
harmonies,  for  the  expression  and  the  artistic  connection  of  a  musical  composition. 
The  external  inducements  for  this  transformation  of  music  were  of  various  kinds. 
First  there  was  the  Protestant  ecclesiastical  chorus.  It  was  a  principle  of  Protes- 
tantism that  the  congregation  itself  should  undertake  the  singing.  But  a  congre- 
gation could  not  be  expected  to  execute  the  artistic  rhythmical  labyrinths  of 
Netherland  polyphony.  On  the  other  hand,  the  founders  of  the  new  confession, 
with  Luther  at  their  head,  were  far  too  penetrated  with  the  power  and  significance 
of  music,  to  reduce  it  at  once  to  an  unadorned  unison.  Hence  the  composers  of 
Protestant  ecclesiastical  music  had  to  solve  the  problem  of  producing  simply 
harmonised  chorales,  in  which  all  the  voices  progressed  at  the  same  time.  This 
excluded  those  canonic  repetitions  of  the  same  melodic  phrases  in  different  parts, 
which  had  hitherto  formed  the  chief  unity  of  the  whole  piece.  A  new  connecting 
principle  had  to  be  looked  for  in  the  sound  of  the  tones  themselves,  and  this  was 

If  found  in  a  stricter  reference  of  all  to  one  predominant  tonic.  The  success  of  this 
problem  was  facilitated  by  the  fact  that  the  Protestant  hymns  were  chiefly  adapted 
to  existing  popular  melodies,  and  the  popular  songs  of  the  Germanic  and  Celtic 
races,  as  already  remarked,  betrayed  a  stricter  feeling  for  tonality  in  the  modem 
sense,  than  those  of  southern  nations.  Thus  as  early  as  in  the  sixteenth  century, 
the  system  of  the  harmony  of  the  ecclesiastical  Ionic  mode  (our  present  major) 
developed  itself  with  tolerable  correctness,  so  that  these  chorales  do  not  strike 
modem  ears  as  strange,  although  they  were  still  without  many  of  our  later  contri> 
vances  for  marking  the  key,  as,  for  example,  the  chord  of  the  dominant  Seventh. 
On  the  other  hand,  it  was  much  longer  before  the  other  ecclesiastical  modes,  in  har- 
monising which  much  uncertainty  still  prevailed,  were  fused  into  the  modem  minor 
mode.  The  Protestant  ecclesiastical  hymns  of  that  time  produced  great  effects 
on  the  feelings  of  contemporaries — a  fact  emphasised  on  all  sides  in  the  liveliest 
language,  so  that  no  doubt  can  exist  that  the  impression  made  by  such  music,  was 
something  as  new  as  it  was  peculiarly  powerful. 

Digitized  by  VjOOQIC 


CHAP.  xm. 


PERIOD  OP  HARMONIC  MUSIC. 


247 


In  the  Roman  Church  also  a  desire  arose  for  altering  their  music.  The  divisions 
of  polyphonic  music  scattered  the  sense  of  the  words,  and  made  them  unintelligible 
to  the  unpractised  public,  and  occasioned  even  a  learned  and  cultivated  hearer  great 
difficulties  in  endeavouring  to  disentangle  the  knot  of  voices.  In  consequence  of 
the  proceedings  of  the  Council  of  Trent,  and  by  an  order  of  Pope  Pius  IV.  (a.d. 
'559-'565)»  Palestrina  (a.d.  1524-1594)  carried  out  this  simplification  and  embel- 
lishment of  ecclesiasticiJ  music,  and  the  simple  beauty  of  his  compositions  is  said  to 
have  prevented  the  complete  banishment  of  part  music  from  the  Roman  liturgy. 
Palestrina,  who  wrote  for  choruses  of  singers  practised  in  their  art,  did  not  entirely 
drop  the  more  complicated  progression  of  parts  found  in  polyphonic  music,  but  by 
appropriate  sections  and  divisions  he  separated  and  connected  both  the  mass  of 
tones  and  the  mass  of  voices,  and  generally  distributed  the  latter  into  several  dis- 
tinct choirs.  The  voices  also  are  more  or  less  frequently  heard  together  in  such 
progressions  as  were  used  in  chorales,  and  in  this  case  consonant  chords  greatly  ^ 
predominated.  By  this  means  he  made  his  pieces  more  comprehensible  and 
intelligible,  and  in  general  extremely  agreeable  to  the  ear.  But  the  deviation  of 
ecolesiastical  modes  from  the  new  modes  invented  in  modern  times  for  the  treat- 
ment of  harmonies,  is  nowhere  so  remarkable  as  in  the  compositions  of  Palestrina 
and  those  of  contemporary  Italian  composers  of  ecclesiastical  music,  among  whom 
Giovanni  Gabrieli,  a  Venetian,  should  be  particularly  named.  Palestrina  was  a 
pupil  of  Claude  Ooudimel  (a  Huguenot,  slain  at  Lyons  in  the  massacre  of  St. 
Bartholomew),  who  had  harmonised  French  psalms  in  a  way  which,  when  the  scale 
was  major,  was  but  very  slightly  different  from  modem  habits.  These  psalm 
melodies  had  been  borrowed,  or  at  least  imitated  from  popular  songs.  Hence  Pales- 
trina was  certainly  acquainted  with  this  mode  of  treatment,  through  his  teacher, 
but  he  had  to  deal  with  themes  from  the  Gregorian  Cantus  firrrms  that  moved  in 
ecclesiastical  tones,  which  he  was  forced  to  maintain  strictly  even  in  pieces  where 
he  himself  invented  or  adapted  the  melodies.  Now  these  modes  necessitated  a  f 
totally  different  harmonic  treatment,  which  sounds  very  strange  to  modems.  As 
a  specimen  I  will  only  cite  the  commencement  of  his  eight-part  Stabat  mater. 


Here,  at  the  commencement  of  a  piece,  just  where  we  should  require  a  steady 
characterisation  of  the  key,  we  find  a  series  of  chords  in  the  most  varied  keys,  ^ 
from  A  major  to  F  major,  apparently  thrown  together  at  haphazard,  contrary  to  all 
our  rules  of  modulation.  What  person  that  was  ignorant  of  ecclesiastical  modes 
could  guess  the  tonic  of  the  piece  from  this  commencement  ?  As  such  we  find  D 
at  the  end  of  the  first  strophe,  and  the  sharpening  of  C  to  (^  in  the  first  chord 
also  points  to  D.  The  principal  melody  too,  which  is  given  to  the  tenor,  shews 
from  the  commencement  that  D  is  the  tonic.  But  we  do  not  get  a  minor  chord  of  D 
till  the  eighth  bar,  whereas  a  modem  composer  would  have  been  forced  to  introduce 
it  in  the  first  good  place  he  could  find  in  the  first  bar. 

We  see  from  these  characters  how  greatly  the  nature  of  the  whole  system  of 
ecclesiastical  modes  differed  from  our  modem  keys.  We  cannot  but  assume  that 
masters  Uke  Palestrina  founded  their  method  of  harmonisation  upon  a  correct  feel- 
ing for  the  peculiar  character  of  those  modes,  and  that,  as  they  could  not  foil  to  be 
acquainted  with  the  contemporary  advances  in  Protestant  ecclesiastical  music, 
their  work  was  neither  arbitrary  nor  unskilful. 


Digitized  by 


Google 


248  PEEIOD  OF  HARMONIC  MUSIC.  part  in. 

WLat  we  miss  in  such  examples  as  the  one  just  adduced,  is  first,  that  the  tonio 
chord  does  not  play  the  same  prominent  part  at  the  very  commencement  that  is 
assigned  to  it  in  modem  music.  In  the  latter,  the  tonic  chord  has  the  same 
prominent  and  connecting  significance  among  chords  as  the  tonic  or  key-note 
among  the  tones  of  the  scale.  Next  we  miss  altogether  that  feeling  for  the  con- 
nection of  consecutive  chords  which  in  modem  times  has  led  to  the  very  general 
custom  of  giving  them  a  common  tone.  This  is  evidently  related  to  the  fact  that, 
as  we  shall  see  hereafter,  it  was  not  possible  in  the  old  ecclesiastical  modes  to 
produce  chains  of  chords  so  closely  connected  with  each  other  and  with  the  tonic 
chord,  as  in  the  modem  major  and  minor  modes. 

Hence,  although  we  recognise  in  Palestnna  and  Oabrieli  a  delicate  artistic 
sensitiveness  for  the  esthetic  effect  of  separate  chords  of  various  kinds,  and  in  so 
^  far  a  certain  independent  significance  in  their  harmonies,  yet  we  see  that  the  means 
of  establishing  an  internal  connection  in  the  tissue  of  chords  had  still  to  be  dis- 
covered. This  problem,  however,  required  a  reduction  and  transformation  of  the 
previous  scales,  to  our  major  and  minor.  On  the  other  hand,  this  reduction 
sacrificed  the  great  variety  of  expression  which  depended  on  diversity  of  scale. 
The  old  scales  partly  form  transitions  between  major  and  minor,  and  partly  enhance 
the  character  of  the  minor,  as  in  the  ecclesiastical  Phrygian  mode  [p.  245^^.  This 
diversity  being  lost,  it  had  to  be  replaced  by  new  contrivances,  such  as  the  trans- 
position of  the  scales  for  different  tonics,  and  the  modulational  passage  from  one 
key  to  another. 

This  transformation  was  completed  during  the  seventeenth  century.  But  the 
most  active  cause  for  ihe  development  of  harmonic  music  is  due  to  the  commence- 
ment of  opera.  Tnis  had  been  occasioned  by  a  revival  of  acquaintance  with 
^  classical  antiquity,  and  its  avowed  object  was  to  rehabilitate  ancient  tragedy,  which 
was  known  to  have  been  recited  musically.  Here  arose  immediately  the  problem 
of  allowing  one  or  two  voices  to  execute  solos ;  but  these  again  had  to  be  harmo* 
nised  so  as  to  fit  in  between  the  chomses,  which  were  treated  in  the  polyphonic 
manner,  the  object  being  to  make  the  solo  parts  stand  prominently  forward  and 
keep  the  accompanying  voices  well  under.  These  conditions  first  gave  rise  to 
Eecitative,  invented  by  Giacomo  Peri  and  Gaccini  in  1600,  and  solo  songs  with 
airs,  invented  by  Claudio  Monteverde  and  Viadana.  The  new  view  taken  of 
harmony  shews  itself  in  written  music  by  the  appearance  of  figured  basses  in  the 
works  of  these  composers.  Every  figured  bass  note  represented  a  chord,  so  that 
the  chords  themselves  were  settled,  but  the  progression  of  the  parts  of  which  they 
were  constituted  was  left  to  the  taste  of  the  player.  And  thus  what  was  merely 
secondary  in  polyphonic  music,  became  principal,  and  conversely. 

Opera  also  necessitated  the  discovery  of  more  powerful  means  of  expression 
f  than  were  admissible  in  ecclesiastical  music.  Monteverde,  who  was  extremely 
prolific  in  inventions,  is  the  first  composer  who  used  chords  of  the  dominant 
Seventh  without  preparation,  for  which  he  was  severely  blamed  by  his  contempo- 
rary Artusi.  Generally  we  find  a  bolder  use  of  dissonances,  which  were  employed 
independently,  to  express  sharp  contrasts  of  expression,  and  not,  as  before,  as 
accidental  results  of  the  progression  of  parts. 

Under  these  influences,  even  as  early  as  in  Monteverde's  time,  the  Doric, 
Eolic,  and  Phrygian  ecclesiastical  modes  [p.  254c,  d]  began  to  be  transformed  and 
fused  into  our  modem  minor  mode.     This  was  completed  in  the  seventeenth  cen 
tury,  and  these  modes  were  thus  made  more  suitable  for  giving  prominence  to  the 
tonic  of  the  harmony,  as  will  be  more  fully  shewn  hereafter. 

We  have  already  given  an  outline  of  the  nature  of  the  influence  which  these 
changes  exerted  on  the  constitution  of  the  tonal  system.  The  mode  of  connecting 
musical  phrases  hitherto  in  vogue— canonic  repetitions  of  similar  melodic  figures 
— had  necessarily  to  be  abandoned  as  soon  as  a  simple  harmonic  accompaniment 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


CHAP.xm.  PERIOD  OF  HARMONIC  MUSIC.  249 

had  to  be  subordinated  to  a  melody.  Hence  some  new  means  of  artistic  connection 
had  to  be  discovered  in  the  somid  of  the  chords  themselves.  This  was  effected, 
first  by  making  the  harmonies  refer  their  tones  much  more  definitely  to  one  pre- 
dominant tonic  than  before,  and  secondly  by  giving  fresh  strength  to  the  rela- 
tions between  the  chords  themselves  and  between  all  other  chords  and  the  tonic 
chord.  In  the  course  of  our  investigations  we  shall  see  that  the  distinctive  pecu- 
liarities of  the  modem  system  of  tones  can  be  deduced  from  this  principle,  and 
that  the  principle  itself  is  very  strictly  carried  out  in  our  present  music.  In 
reality  the  mode  in  which  the  materials  of  music  are  now  worked  up  for  artistic 
use,  is  in  itself  a  wondrous  work  of  art,  at  which  the  experience,  ingenuity,  and 
esthetic  feeling  of  European  nations  has  laboured  for  between  two  and  three  thou- 
sand years,  since  the  days  of  Terpander  and  Pythagoras.  But  the  complete  for- 
mation of  the  essential  features  as  we  now  see  it,  is  scarcely  two  hundred  years 
old  in  the  practice  of  musical  composers,  and  theoretical  expression  was  not  given  ^ 
to  the  new  principle  till  the  time  of  Rameau  at  the  beginning  of  last  century.  In 
the  historical  point  of  view,  therefore,  it  is  wholly  the  product  of  modem  times, 
limited  nationally  to  the  German,  Roman,  Celtic,  and  Sclavonic  races. 

With  this  tonal  system,  which  admits  great  wealth  of  form  with  strictly  defined 
artistic  consistency,  it  has  become  possible  to  construct  works  of  art,  of  much 
greater  extent,  and  much  richer  in  forms  and  parts,  much  more  energetic  in 
expression,  than  any  producible  in  past  ages ;  and  hence  we  are  by  no  means 
inclined  to  quarrel  with  modem  musicians  for  esteeming  it  the  best  of  all,  and 
devoting  their  attention  to  it  exclusively.  But  scientifically,  when  we  proceed  to 
explain  its  contitraction  and  display  its  consistency  we  must  not  forget  that  our 
modem  system  was  not  developed  from  a  natural  necessity,  but  from  a  freely 
chosen  principle  of  style  ;  that  beside  it,  and  before  it,  other  tonal  systems  have 
been  developed  from  other  principles,  and  that  in  each  such  system  the  highest 
pitch  of  artistic  beauty  has  been  reached,  by  the  successful  solution  of  more  % 
limited  problems. 

This  reference  to  the  history  of  music  was  necessitated  by  our  inability  in  this 
case  to  appeal  to  observation  and  experiment  for  establishing  our  explanations, 
because,  educated  in  a  modem  system  of  music,  we  cannot  thoroughly  throw  our- 
selves back  into  the  condition  of  our  ancestors,  who  knew  nothing  about  what  we 
have  been  familiar  with  from  childhood,  and  who  had  to  find  it  all  out  for  them- 
selves. The  only  observations  and  experiments,  therefore,  to  which  we  can  appeal, 
are  those  which  mankind  themselves  have  undertaken  in  the  development  of  music. 
If  our  theory  of  the  modem  tonal  system  is  correct  it  must  also  suffice  to  furnish 
the  requisite  explanation  of  the  former  less  perfect  stages  of  development. 

As  the  fundamental  principle  for  the  development  of  the  European  tonal 
system,  we  shall  assume  that  the  whole  mass  of  tones  and  the  connection  of  har- 
monies must  stand  in  a  close  and  always  distinctly  perceptible  relationship  to  soms 
arbitrarily  selected  tonic,  and  that  the  inass  of  tone  which  forms  the  whole  compo-  % 
sition,  must  be  developed  from  this  tome,  and  mv^t  finally  return  to  it.  The 
ancient  world  developed  this  principle  in  homophonic  music,  the  modem  world  in 
harmonic  music.  But  it  is  evident  that  this  is  merely  an  esthetical  principle,  not 
A  natural  law. 

The  correctness  of  this  principle  cannot  be  established  d  priori.    It  must 
be  tested  by  its  results.    The  origin  of  such  esthetical  principles  should  not  be     » 
ascribed  to  a  natural  necessity.    They  are  the  inventions  of  genius,  as  we  previously 
endeavoured  to  illustrate  by  a  reference  to  the  principles  of  architectural  style. 


Digitized  by 


Google 


aso  PKOGKESSION  BY  INTERVALS.  pabt  in. 

CHAPTER  XTV. 

THE  TONALITY  OP  HOMOPHONIC  MUSIC. 

Musio  was  forced  first  to  select  artistically,  and  then  to  shape  for  itself,  the  material 
on  which  it  works.  Painting  and  sculpture  find  the  fundamental  character  of 
their  materials,  form  and  colour,  in  nature  itself,  which  they  strive  to  imitate. 
Poetry  finds  its  material  ready  formed  in  the  words  of  language.  Architecture 
has,  indeed,  also  to  create  its  own  forms ;  but  they  are  partly  forced  upon  it  by 
technical  and  not  by  purely  artistic  considerations.  Music  alone  finds  an  infi- 
nitely rich  but  totally  shapeless  plastic  material  in  the  tones  of  the  human  voice  and 
artificial  musical  instruments,  which  must  be  shaped  on  purely  artistic  principles, 
unfettered  by  any  reference  to  utility  as  in  architecture,  or  to  the  imitation  of 

^  nature  as  in  the  fine  arts,  or  to  the  existing  symbolical  meaning  of  sounds  as  in 
poetry. .  There  is  a  greater  and  more  absolute  freedom  in  the  use  of  the  material 
for  music  than  for  any  other  of  the  arts.  But  certainly  it  is  more  difficult  to  make 
a  proper  use  of  absolute  fi:eedom,than  to  advance  where  external  irremovable  land- 
marks limit  the  width  of  the  path  which  the  artist  has  to  traverse.  Hence  also 
the  cultivation  of  the  tonal  material  of  music  has,  as  we  have  seen,  proceeded  much 
more  slowly  than  the  development  of  the  other  arts. 
It  is  now  our  business  to  investigate  this  cultivation. 

The  first  feust  that  we  meet  with  in  the  music  of  all  nations,  so  £Eur  as  is  yet 
known,  is  that  alterations  of  pitch  in  melodies  take  place  by  intervals^  and  not  by 
contimums  transitions.  The  psychological  reason  of  this  fact  would  seem  to  be 
the  same  as  that  which  led  to  rhythmic  subdivision  periodically  repeated.  All 
melodies  are  motions  within  extremes  of  pitch.  The  incorporeal  material  of  tones 
is  much  more  adapted  for  following  the  musician's  intention  in  the  most  delicate 

^  and  phant  maimer  for  every  species  of  motion,  than  any  corporeal  material  how- 
ever light.  Graceful  rapidity,  grave  procession,  quiet  advance,  wild  leaping,  all 
these  different  characters  of  motion  and  a  thousand  others  in  the  most  varied 
combinations  and  degrees,  can  be  represented  by  successions  of  tones.  And  as 
music  expresses  these  motions,  it  gives  an  expression  also  to  those  mental  con- 
ditions which  naturally  evoke  similar  motions,  whether  of  the  body  and  the  voice,  or 
of  the  thinking  and  feeling  principle  itself.  Every  motion  is  an  expression  of  the 
power  which  produces  it,  and  we  instinctively  measure  the  motive  force  by  the 
amount  of  motion  which  it  produces.  This  holds  equally  and  perhaps  more  for  the 
motions  due  to  the  exertion  of  power  by  the  human  will  and  human  impulses, 
than  for  the  mechanical  motions  of  external  nature.  In  this  way  melodic  pro- 
gression can  become  the  expression  of  the  most  diverse  conditions  of  human  dis- 
position, not  precisely  of  hximei^a  feelings  *  but  at  least  of  that  state  of  sensitiven>ess 
which  is  produced  by  feelings.     In  English  the  phrase  out  of  tune,  unstrung j  and 

^  in  German  the  word  stimmung,  literally  tuning,  are  transferred  from  music  to 
mental  states.  The  words  are  meant  to  denote  those  peculiarities  of  mental  con- 
dition which  are  capable  of  musical  representation.  I  think  we  might  appro- 
priately define  gemilthsstimmung,  or  mentaZ  tune,  as  representing  that  general  cha- 
racter temporarily  shewn  by  the  motion  of  our  conceptions,  and  correspondingly 
impressed  on  the  motions  of  our  body  and  voice.  Our  thoughts  may  move  fa*st  or 
slowly,  may  wander  about  restlessly  and  aimlessly  in  anxious  excitement,  or  may 
keep  a  determinate  aim  distinctly  and  energetically  in  view;  they  may  lounge 
about  without  care  or  effort  in  pleasant  fancies,  or,  driven  back  by  some  sad 
memories,  may  return  slowly  and  heavily  from  the  spot  with  short  weak  steps. 
All  this  may  be  imitated  and  expressed  by  the  melodic  motion  of  the  tones,  and 
the  listener  may  thus  receive  a  more  perfect  and  impressive  image  of  the '  tune '  of 

*  Hanslick  seems  to  me  to  have  the  advan-      means  of  clearly  oharacterising  the  object  ol 
tage  over  other  esthetic  writers  in  this  point,      feeling, 
because  music,  unassisted  by  poetry,  has  no 


Digitized  by 


Google 


CHAP.  XIV.  PROGEESSION  BY  INTERVALS.  251 

another  person's  mind,  than  by  any  other  means,  except  perhaps  by  a  very  perfect 
dramatic  representation  of  the  way  in  which  such  a  person  really  spoke  and  acted. 

Aristotle  also  formed  a  similar  conception  of  the  effect  of  music.  In  his  29th 
problem  he  says :  *  Why  do  rhythms  and  melodies,  which  are  composed  of  sound, 
resemble  the  feelings ;  while  this  is  not  the  case  for  tastes,  colours,  or  smells  ?  Can 
it  be  because  they  are  motions,  as  actions  are  also  motions  ?  Energy  itself  belongs 
to  feeling  and  creates  feeling.  But  tastes  and  colours  do  not  act  in  the  same  way.'  * 
And  at  the  end  of  the  27th  problem  he  says  :  *  These  motions,  i.e.  rhythms  and 
melodies,  are  active,  and  action  is  the  sign  of  feeling.'  f 

Not  merely  music  but  even  other  kinds  of  motions  may  produce  similar  effects. 
Water  in  motion,  as  in  cascades  or  sea  waves,  has  an  effect  in  some  respects 
similar  to  music.  How  long  and  how  often  can  we  sit  and  look  at  the  waves 
rolling  in  to  shore  I  Their  rhythmic  motion,  perpetually  varied  in  detail,  produces 
a  peculiar  feeling  of  pleasant  repose  or  weariness,  and  l^e  impression  of  a  mighty  V 
orderly  life,  finely  linked  together.  When  the  sea  is  quiet  and  smooth  we  can 
enjoy  its  colouring  for  a  while,  but  this  gives  no  such  lasting  pleasure  as  the  rolling 
waves.  Small  undulations,  on  the  other  hand,  on  small  surfaces  of  water,  follow 
one  another  too  rapidly,  and  disturb  rather  than  please. 

But  the  motion  of  tone  surpasses  all  motion  of  corporeal  masses  in  the  delicacy 
and  ease  with  which  ;t  can  receive  and  imitate  the  most  varied  descriptions  of 
expression.  Hence  it  arrogates  to  itself  by  right  the  representation  of  states  of 
mind,  which  the  other  arts  can  only  indirectly  touch  by  shewing  the  situations 
which  caused  the  emotion,  or  by  giving  the  resulting  words,  acts,  or  outward 
appearance  of  the  body.  The  union  of  music  to  words  is  most  important,  because 
words  can  represent  the  cause  of  the  frame  of  mind,  the  object  to  which  it  refers, 
and  the  feeling  which  lies  at  its  root,  while  music  expresses  the  kind  of  mental 
transition  which  is  due  to  the  feeling.  When  different  hearers  endeavour  to  de- 
scribe the  impression  of  instrumental  music,  they  often  adduce  entirely  different  % 
situations  or  feelings  which  they  suppose  to  have  been  symbolised  by  the  music. 
One  who  knows  nothing  of  the  matter  is  then  very  apt  to  ridicule  such  enthusiasts, 
and  yet  they  may  have  been  all  more  or  less  right,  because  music  does  not  represent 
feelings  and  situations,  but  only  frames  of  mind,  which  the  hearer  is  unable  to 
describe  except  by  adducing  such  outward  circumstances  as  he  has  himself  noticed 
when  experiencing  the  corresponding  mental  states.  Now  different  feelings  may 
occur  under  different  circumstances  and  produce  the  same  states  of  mind  in  dif- 
ferent individuals,  while  the  same  feelings  may  give  rise  to  different  states  of  mind. 
Love  is  a  feeling.  But  music  cannot  represent  it  directly  as  such.  The  mental 
states  of  a  lover  may,  as  we  know,  shew  the  extremest  variety  of  change.  Now 
music  may  perhaps  express  the  dreamy  longing  for  transcendent  bhss  which  love 

*  Aid  ri  ol  pvBfxoi  Kcd  rh,  fA4\ri  ^mvif  qIvo,  the  only  sensation  which  excites  the  feelings  ? 

'f^ctf'u^  loucer  •  •/  Hk    x^f^^  ^^*  ^^^'    ^^^^  '''^  Even  melody  without  words  has  feeling.    But 

Xp^fwra  Kcd  €d6(rfiai;*H6riKurfi(r€is€Ur\y/&ffir€p  this  is  not  the  case  for  colour,  or  smell,  or  ^ 

Kcd  al  trpd^tis ;  ffiri  Z\  ^  fiky  Mpytta  ii9uchy,  koL  taste.    Is  it  because  they  have  none  of  the 

woui  liBos'  ol  9h  X"f">^  *^^  "^^  XP^M^'''^  ^^  iroiovffiy  motion  which  sound  excites  in  us  ?    For  the 

Afioiws,    Arist.  Prob,  xix.  29.  others  excite  motion  ;  thus  colour  moves  the 

t  [The  above  words  conclude  the  problem,  eye.    But  we  feel  the  motion  which  follows 

which  it  seems  best  to  cite  in  full.    Aiii  r(  rh  sound.    And  this  is  alike,  in  rhythm,  and 

iucovarhv  lUvov  ^dos  Ixci  r&v  ataBrrruy ;  kc^  7^  alteration  in  pitch,  but  not  in  united  sounds. 

ihf  f  &ycv  kj6yov  fA4\os,  ZfjL»t  %x^i  ^Bor  &AX*  ob  Sounding  notes  together  does  not  excite  feel- 

T^  Xpif/Mf  oitik  ^  ifffiii,  ov9h  6  xvfi^f  ^X<t«    *H  ing.    This  is  not  the  case  for  other  sensations. 

8ti  Klr/iffiu  ^x^i  iiovovovx^i  hv  ^  ^6<bos  ii/xas  Kivti;  Kow  these  motions  stimulate  action,  and  this 

Totwdrri  fjL^y  yikp  Kcd  rois  JixkoLs  iiripxfi,  fctyti  ykp  action  is  the  sign  of  feeling.'    Aristotle  seems 

«a2  T^  Xf^MA  [f^oX]  r^y  i^iy  ■  tiXAA  rris  ivofitimis  to  have  required  motion  to  excite  feeling,  and 

T^  TQio^^  ^^^  tdada»6tA§$a  Kty4ia9ms,    ASrti  in  sounding  two  notes  together,  there  was  no 

9i  tx^i  SfioUrnrOf  Hy  re  rots  fve/iois  koX  4y  rp  motion  of  one  towards  the  other.    It  is  evi- 

T&y  ^$6yywy  rd^ti  r&y  6^4»y  xai  fiapimy,  o^k  iy  dent  that  he  had  not  the  slightest  inkling  of  a 

T$  /tf|ci.     *AAA'  ii  ifvfA^yia  ovk  Ix^'  ^^'-     '^'^  progression  of  /larmomes,  and  this  utter  blank 

Si  Toif  &AAOU  al(r07irois  rovro  oIk  forty.    Ai  8i  in  his  mind  is  one  of  the  strongest  proofs  that 

KUfiiTtts  o^rai,  itptutrtKai  flaiy.  At  9^  irpd^tis,  ijBovs  the  Greeks  had  never  tried  harmony.    *Apfioyia 

irriiutMrta  i<rrl.    Arist.  Prob,  xix.  27.    Which  we  had  the  modem  meaning  of  melody ;  fi€\<f9ia 

may  perhaps  translate  thus :  '  Why  is  sound  was  words  set  to  mvLOC.— Translator,] 


Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


252  PROGRESSION  BY  INTERVALS.  pabt  in. 

may  excite.  But  precisely  the  same  state  of  mind  might  arise  from  religious 
enthusiasm.  Hence  when  a  piece  of  music  expresses  this  mental  state  it  is  not 
a  contradiction  for  one  hearer  to  find  in  it  the  longing  of  love,  and  another  the 
longing  of  enthusiastic  piety.  In  this  sense  Vischer's  rather  paradoxical  state- 
ment that  the  mechanics  of  mental  emotion  are  perhaps  best  studied  in  their 
musical  expression,  may  be  not  altogether  incorrect.  We  really  possess  no  other 
means  of  expressing  them  so  exactly  and  deUcately. 

As  we  have  seen,  then,  melody  has  to  express  a  motion,  in  such  a  manner  that 
the  hearer  may  easily,  clearly,  and  certainly  appreciate  the  character  of  that  motion 
by  immediate  perception.  This  is  only  possible  when  the  steps  of  this  motion, 
their  rapidity  and  their  amount,  are  also  exactly  measurable  by  iromediate  sensible 
perception.  Melodic  motion  is  change  of  pitch  in  time.  To  measure  it  perfectly, 
the  length  of  time  elapsed,  and  the  distance  between  the  pitches,  must  be  measur- 

^  able.  This  is  possible  for  iromediate  audition  only  on  condition  that  the  altera- 
tions both  in  time  and  pitch  should  proceed  by  regular  and  determinate  degrees. 
This  is  immediately  clear  for  time,  for  even  the  scientific,  as  well  as  all  other 
measurement  of  time,  depends  on  the  rhythmical  recurrence  of  similar  events,  the 
revolution  of  the  earth  or  moon,  or  the  swings  of  a  pendulum.  Thus  also  the 
regular  alternation  of  accentuated  and  unaccentuated  sounds  in  music  and  poetry 
gives  the  measure  of  time  for  the  composition.  But  whereas  in  poetry  the  con- 
struction of  the  verse  serves  only  to  reduce  the  external  accidents  of  linguistic 
expression  to  artistic  order ;  in  music,  rhythm,  as  the  measure  of  time,  belongs  to 
the  inmost  nature  of  expression.  Hence  also  a  much  more  delicate  and  elaborate 
development  of  rhythm  was  required  in  music  than  in  verse. 

It  was  also  necessary  that  the  alteration  of  pitch  should  proceed  by  intervals, 
because  motion  is  not  measurable  by  immediate  perception  unless  the  amount  of 
space  to  be  measured  is  divided  off  into  degrees.    Even  in  scientific  investigations 

^  we  are  unable  to  measure  the  velocity  of  continuous  motion  except  by  comparing 
the  space  described  with  the  standard  measure,  as  we  compare  time  with  the  seconds 
pendulum. 

It  may  be  objected  that  architecture  in  its  arabesques,  which  have  been 
justly  compared  in  many  respects  with  musical  figures,  and  which  also  shew 
a  certain  orderly  arrangement,  constantly  employs  curved  lines  and  not  lines 
broken  into  determinate  lengths.  But  in  the  first  place  the  art  of  arabesques  really 
began  with  the  Greek  meander,  which  is  composed  of  straight  lines  set  at  right 
angles  to  each  other,  following  at  exactly  equal  lengths,  and  cutting  one  another 
off  in  degrees.  In  the  second  place,  the  eye  which  contemplates  arabesques  can 
take  in  and  compare  all  parts  of  the  curved  lines  at  once,  and  can  glance  to  and 
fro,  and  return  to  its  first  contemplation.  Hence,  notwithstanding  the  continuous 
curvature  of  the  lines,  their  paths  are  perfectly  comprehensible,  and  it  became 
possible  to  renounce  the  strict  regularity  of  the  Grecian  arabesques  in  favour  of 

^  the  curvilinear  freedom.  But  whilst  freer  forms  are  thus  admitted  for  individual 
small  decorations  in  architecture,  the  division  of  any  great  whole,  whether  it  be  a 
series  of  arabesques  or  a  row  of  windows  or  colunms,  &c.,  throughout  a  building,  is 
st'U  tied  down  to  the  simple  arithmetical  law  of  repetition  of  similar  parts  at  equal 
intervals. 

The  individual  parts  of  a  melody  reach  the  ear  in  succession.  We  cannot  per- 
ceive them  all  at  once.  We  cannot  observe  backwards  and  forwards  at  pleasure. 
Hence  for  a  clear  and  sure  measurement  of  the  change  of  pitch,  no  means  was 
left  but  progression  by  determinate  degrees.  This  series  of  degrees  is  laid  down 
in  the  musical  scale.  When  the  wind  howls  and  its  pitch  rises  or  falls  in  insensible 
gradations  without  any  break,  we  have  nothing  to  measure  the  variations  of  pitch, 
nothing  by  which  we  can  compare  the  later  with  the  earlier  sounds,  and  compre- 
hend the  extent  of  the  change.  The  whole  phenomenon  produces  a  confused,  un- 
pleasant impression.  The  musical  scale  is  as  it  were  the  divided  rod,  by  which  we 
measure  progression  in  pitch,  as  rhythm  measures  progression  in  time.     Hence 

Digitized  by  V^OOQIC 


CHAP.  XIV.    RATIONAL  CONSTRUCTION  OF  DIATONIC  SCALES.      253, 

the  analogy  between  the  scale  of  tones  and  rhythm  naturally  occurred  to  musical 
theoreticians  of  ancient  as  well  as  modem  times. 

We  consequently  find  the  most  complete  agreement  among  all  nations  that  use 
music  at  all,  from  the  earliest  to  the  latest  times,  as  to  the  separation  of  certain 
determinate  degrees  of  tone  from  the  possible  mass  of  continuous  gradations 
of  sound,  all  of  which  are  audible,  and  these  degrees  form  the  scale  in  which 
the  melody  moves.  But  in  selecting  the  particular  degrees  of  pitch,  deviations 
of  national  taste  become  inunediately  apparent.  The  number  of  scales  used  by 
different  nations  and  at  different  times  is  by  no  means  small. 

Let  us  inquire,  then,  what  motive  there  can  be  for  selecting  one  tone  rather 
than  another  in  its  neighbourhood  for  the  step  succeeding  any  given  tone.  We 
remember  that  in  sounding  two  tones  together  such  a  relation  was  observed.  We 
found  that  under  such  circumstances  certain  particular  intervals,  namely  the  con- 
sonances, were  distinguished  from  all  other  intervals  which  were  nearly  the  same,  ^ 
by  the  absence  of  beats.  Now  some  of  these  intervals,  the  Octave,  Fifth,  and 
Fourth,  are  found  in  all  the  musical  scales  known.*  Recent  theoreticians  that 
have  been  bom  and  bred  in  the  system  of  harmonic  music,  have  consequently 
supposed  that  they  could  explain  the  origin  of  the  scales,  by  the  assumption  that 
all  melodies  arise  from  thinking  of  a  harmony  to  them,  and  that  the  scale  itself, 
considered  as  the  melody  of  the  key,  arose  from  resolving  the  fundamental  chords 
of  the  key  into  their  separate  tones.  This  view  is  certainly  correct  for  modem 
scales ;  at  least  these  have  been  modified  to  suit  the  requirements  of  the  harmony. 
But  scales  existed  long  before  there  was  any  knowledge  or  experience  of  harmony  at 
all.  And  when  we  see  historically  what  a  long  period  of  time  musicians  required 
to  learn  how  to  accompany  a  melody  by  harmonies,  and  how  awkward  their  first 
attempts  were,  we  cannot  feel  a  doubt  that  ancient  composers  had  no  feeling  at  all 
for  harmonic  accompaniment,  just  as  even  at  the  present  day  many  of  the  more 
gifted  Orientals  are  opposed  to  our  own  harmonic  music.  We  must  also  not  forget  ^ 
that  many  popular  melodies,  of  older  times  or  foreign  origin,  scarcely  admit  of  any 
harmonic  accompaniment  at  all,  without  injury  to  their  character. 

The  same  remark  applies  to  Rameau's  assumption  of  an  *  understood '  funda- 
mental bass  in  the  constraction  of  melodies  or  scales  for  a  single  voice.  A  modern 
composer  would  certainly  imagine  to  himself  at  once  tlie  fundamental  bass  to  the 
melody  he  invents.  But  how  could  that  be  the  case  with  musicians  who  had  never 
heard  any  harmonic  music,  and  had  no  idea  how  to  compose  any  ?  Granted  that 
an  artist's  genius  often  unconsciously  *  feels  out  *  many  relations,  we  should  be 
imputing  too  much  to  it  if  we  asserted  that  the  artist  could  observe  relations  of 
tones  which  he  had  never  or  very  rarely  heard,  and  which  were  destined  not  to  be 
discovered  and  employed  till  many  centuries  after  his  time. 

It  is  clear  that  in  the  period  of  homophonic  music,  the  scale  could  not  have 
been  constructed  so  as  to  suit  the  requirements  of  chordal  connections  uncon- 
sciously supplied.  Yet  a  meaning  may  be  assigned,  in  a  somewhat  altered  form,  ^ 
to  the  views  and  hypotheses  of  musicians  above  mentioned,  by  supposing  that  the 
same  physical  and  physiological  relations  of  the  tones,  which  become  sensible 
when  they  are  sounded  together  and  determine  the  magnitude  of  the  consonlint 
intervals,  might  also  have  had  an  effect  in  the  construction  of  the  scale,  although 
under  somewhat  different  circumstances. 

Let  us  begin  with  the  Octave,  in  which  the  relationship  to  the  fundamental  tone 
is  most  remarkable.  Let  any  melody  be  executed  on  any  instrument  which  has  a 
good  musical  quahty  of  tone,  such  as  a  human  voice ;  the  hearer  must  have  heard 
not  only  the  primes  of  the  compound  tones,  but  also  their  upper  Octaves,  and,  less 
strongly,  the  remaining  upper  partials.  When,  then,  a  higher  voice  afterwards 
executes  the  same  melody  an  Octave  higher,  we  hear  again  a  part  of  what  we 
heard  before,  namely  the  evenly  numbered  partial  tones  (p.  49^^)  of  the  former 

*  [It  will  be  seen  in  App.  XX.  sect.  E.  that  the  Fourth  and  Fifth  are  often  materially  inexact 
or  designedly  eMered,  -Traiislator,] 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


254  RELATIONSHIP  OF  COMPOUND  TONES.  part  in. 

compound  tones,  and  at  the  same  time  we  hear  nothing  that  we  had  not  previously 
heard.  Hence  the  repetition  of  a  melody  in  the  higher  Octave  is  a  real  repetition 
of  what  has  been  previously  heard,  not  of  all  of  it,  but  of  a  part.*  If  we  allow  a 
low  voice  to  be  accompanied  by  a  higher  in  the  Octave  above  it,  the  only  part 
music  which  the  Greeks  employed,  we  add  nothing  new,  we  merely  reinforce  the 
evenly  numbered  partials.  In  this  sense,  then,  the  compound  tones  of  an  Octave 
above  are  really  repetitions  of  the  tones  of  the  lower  Octaves,  or  at  least  of  part  of 
their  constituents.  Hence  the  first  and  chief  division  of  our  musical  scale  is  that  into 
a  series  of  Octaves.  In  reference  to  both  melody  and  harmony,  we  assume  tones 
of  different  Octaves  which  bear  the  same  name,  to  have  the  same  value,  and,  in  the 
sense  intended,  and  up  to  a  certain  point,  this  assumption  is  correct.  An  accom- 
paniment of  Octaves  gives  perfect  consonance,  but  it  gives  nothing  additional ;  it 
merely  reinforces  tones  already  present.  Hence  it  is  musically  applicable  for  in- 
%  creasing  the  power  of  a  melody  which  has  to  be  brought  out  strongly,  but  it  has 
none  of  the  variety  of  polyphonic  music,  and  therefore  is  felt  to  be  monotonous, 
and  it  is  consequently  forbidden  in  polyphonic  music. 

What  is  true  of  the  Octave  is  true  in  a  less  degree  for  the  Twelfth.  11  a  melody 
is  repeated  in  the  Twelfth  we  again  hear  only  what  we  had  already  heard,  but  the 
repeated  part  of  what  we  heard  is  much  weaker,  because  only  the  third,  sixth, 
ninth,  &c.,  partial  tone  is  repeated,  whereas  for  repetition  in  the  Octave,  instead  of 
the  third  partial,  the  much  stronger  second  and  weaker  fourth  partial  is  heard,  and 
in  place  of  the  ninth,  the  eighth  and  tenth  occur,  &c.  Hence  repetition  of  a 
melody  in  the  Twelfth  is  less  complete  than  repetition  in  the  Octave,  because  only 
a  smaller  part  of  what  had  been  already  heard  is  repeated.  In  place  of  this 
repetition  in  the  Twelfth,  we  may  substitute  one  an  Octave  lower,  namely  in  the 
Fifth.  Bepetition  in  the  Fifth  is  not  a  pure  repetition,  as  that  in  the  Twelfth  is. 
Taking  2  for  the  pitch  number  of  the  prime  tone,  the  partials  are  (197c,  d) 

H              for  the  fundamental  compound        .2  4  6    S        1012- 

for  the  Twelfth       ....  6  12 

for  the  Fifth 3  69  12 

When  we  strike  the  Twelfth  we  repeat  the  simple  tones  6  and  12,  which  already 
existed  in  the  fundamental  compound  tone.  When  we  strike  the  Fifth,  we  continue 
to  repeat  the  same  simple  tones,  but  we  also  add  two  others,  3  and  9.  Hence  for 
the  repetition  in  the  Fifth,  only  a  part  of  the  new  sound  is  identical  with  a  part  of 
what  had  been  heard,  but  it  is  nevertheless  the  most  perfect  repetition  which  can  be 
executed  at  a  smaller  interval  than  an  Octave.  This  is  clearly  the  reason  why 
unpractised  singers,  when  they  wish  to  join  in  the  chorus  to  a  song  that  does  not 
suit  the  compass  of  their  voice,  often  take  a  Fifth  to  it.  This  is  also  a  very  evident 
proof  that  the  uncultivated  ear  regards  repetition  in  the  Fifth  as  natur^J.  Such 
an  accompaniment  in  the  Fifth  and  Fourth  is  said  to  have  been  systematically 
developed  in  the  early  part  of  the  middle  ages.  Even  in  modem  music,  repetition 
II  in  the  Fifth  plays  a  prominent  part  next  to  repetition  in  the  Octave.  In  normal 
fugues  the  theme,  as  is  well  known,  is  first  repeated  in  the  Fifth ;  in  the  normal 
form  of  instrumental  pieces,  that  of  the  Sonata,  the  theme  in  the  first  movement 
is  transposed  to  the  Fifth,  returning  in  the  second  part  to  the  fundamental  tone. 
This  kind  of  imperfect  repetition  of  the  impression  in  the  Fifth  induced  the  Greeks 
also  to  divide  the  interval  of  the  Octave  into  two  equivalent  sections,  namely  two 
Tctrachords.    Our  major  scale  on  being  divided  in  this  manner  would  be : — 


d     e     f       g     a     b     c'    d'    e'     f 


II.  m. 


*  [Some  considerations  have  been  omitted,      powerful  as  in  the  higher  tone.    The  upper 
probably  by  design.    The  quality  of  tone  of      partials  of  the  higher  tone,  which  are  still 
the  Yoice  which    sings  the  Octave  above  is      quite  effective,  would  be  inaudible  in  the  lower 
materially  different.     The   evenly  numbered      tone. — Translator.'] 
partials  of  the  lower  tone  are  by  no  means  so 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


CHAP.  XIV.  RELATIONSHIP  OF  COMPOUND  TONES.  255 

The  suocession  of  tones  in  the  second  tetrachord  is  a  repetition  of  that  in  the 
first,  transposed  a  Fifth.*  To  pass  into  the  Octave  division,  the  successive  tetra- 
chords  must  be  alternately  separate  and  connected.  They  are  said  to  be  connected, 
or  conjunct,  when,  as  in  II.  and  III.,  the  last  tone  c  of  the  lower  becomes  the  first 
of  the  higher  tetrachord ;  and  separate,  or  disjunct,  when,  as  in  I.  and  11.,  the  last 
tone  of  the  lower  is  different  from  the  upper.  In  the  second  tetrachord  ^  to  0,  every 
ascending  series  of  tones  necessarily  leads  to  d  as  the  final  tone,  and  this  c'  is  also 
the  Octave  of  the  fundamental  tone  of  the  first  tetrachord.  Now  this  &  is  the  Fourth 
of  g,  the  fundamental  tone  of  the  second  tetrachord.  To  make  the  succession  of 
tones  the  same  in  both  tetrachords,  the  lower  tetrachord  had  to  be  increased  by  the 
tone  /  which  answers  to  c'.  The  Fourth  /,  however,  would  have  suggested  itself 
in  the  same  way  as  the  Fifth,  independently  of  this  analogy  of  the  tetrachords. 
The  Fifth  is  a  compound  tone  in  which  the  second  partial  is  the  third  partial  of  the 
fundamental  compound  tone  ;  the  Fourth  is  a  compound  tone  in  which  the  third  ^ 
partial  is  the  same  as  the  second  of  the  Octave.  Hence  the  limits  of  the  two 
analogous  divisions  of  the  Octave  are  settled,  namely  : — 

c— /,       g  —  c, 

but  the  mode  of  filling  up  these  gaps  remains  arbitrary,  and  different  plans  for 
doing  so  were  adopted  by  the  Greeks  themselves  at  different  periods,  and  others 
again  by  other  nations.  But  the  division  of  the  scale  into  octaves,  and  the  octave 
into  two  analogous  tetrachords,  occurs  everywhere,  almost  without  exception. 

Boethius  (De  Musica,  lib.  i.  cap.  20)  informs  us  that  according  to  Nicomachus 
the  most  ancient  method  of  tuning  the  lyre  down  to  the  time  of  Orpheus,  con- 
sisted of  open  tetrachords, 

Q—f—g  —  d, 

with  which  certainly  it  was  scarcely  possible  to  construct  a  melody.    But  as  it  ^ 
contained  the  chief  degrees  of  the  pitch  of  ordinary  speech,  a  lyre  of  this  kind 
might  possibly  have  served  to  accompany  declamation. 

The  relationship  of  the  Fifth,  and  its  inversion  the  Fourth,  to  the  fundamental 
tone,  is  so  close  that  it  has  been  acknowledged  in  all  known  systems  of  music.f 
On  the  other  hand,  many  variations  occur  in  the  choice  of  the  intermediate  tones 
which  have  to  be  inserted  between  the  terminal  tones  of  the  tetrachord.  The 
interval  of  a  Third  is  by  no  means  so  clearly  defined  by  easily  appreciable  partial 
tones,  as  to  have  forced  itself  from  the  first  on  the  ear  of  unpractised  musicians. 
We  must  remember  that  even  if  the  fifth  partial  tone  existed  in  the  compound 
tones  of  the  musical  instruments  employed,  it  would  have  had  to  contend  with  the 
much  louder  prime  tone,  and  would  also  have  been  covered  by  the  three  adjacent 
and  lower  partials.  As  a  matter  of  fact,  the  history  of  musical  systems  shews  that 
there  was  much  and  long  hesitation  as  to  the  tuning  of  the  Thirds.  And  the  doubt 
is  even  yet  felt  when  Thirds  are  used  in  pure  melody,  unconnected  with  any  har-  fl 
monies.  I  must  own  that  on  observing  isolated  intervals  of  this  kind,  I  cannot 
come  to  perfectly  certain  results,  but  I  do  so  when  I  hear  them  in  a  well-constructed 
melody  with  distinct  tonahty.  The  natural  major  Thirds  of  4  :  5  thus  seem  to  me 
calmer  and  quieter  than  the  sharper  major  Thirds  of  our  equally  tempered  modern 
instruments,  or  with  the  still  sharper  major  Thirds  which  result  from  the  Pytha- 
gorean tuning  with  perfect  Fifths.  Both  of  the  latter  intervals  have  a  strained 
effect.  Most  of  our  modem  musicians,  accustomed  to  the  major  Thirds  of  the 
equal  temperament,  prefer  them  to  the  perfect  major  Thirds,  when  melody  alone  is 
concerned.  But  I  have  convinced  myself  that  artists  of  the  first  rank,  like  Joachim, 
use  the  Thirds  of  4  :  5  even  in  melody.    For  harmony  there  is  no  doubt  at  all. 

*  [This  applies  to  the  Pythagorean  scale      whereas  ^  to  a  is  a  minor  Tone  and  a  to  5  a 
and  hence  to  Greek  music,  and  also  to  all      major  Tone.  These  distinctions  were  of  coarse 
tempered  music.    But  in  just  intonation  c  to  (2       purposely  omitted  in  the  text. — Translator.] 
is  a  major  Tone,  and  d  to  e  &  minor  Tone,  f  C^^t  see  App.  XX.  sect.  K,— Translator,] 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


256  RELATIONSHIP  OP  COMPOUND  TONES.  part  iii. 

Every  one  chooses  the  natural  major  Thirds.    In  Chapter  XVI.  I  shall  describe  on 
instrument  which  will  enable  any  one  to  perform  experiments  of  this  kind.* 

Under  these  circumstances  another  principle  for  determining  the  small  intervals 
of  the  scale  was  resorted  to  during  the  in&ncy  of  music,  and  seems  to  be  still 
employed  among  the  less  civilised  nations.  This  principle,  which  has  subsequently 
had  to  yield  to  that  of  tonal  relationship,  consists  in  an  endeavour  to  distinguish 
equal  intervals  by  ear,  and  thus  make  the  differences  of  pitch  perceptibly  uni- 
form. 

This  attempt  has  never  prevailed  over  the  feeling  of  tonal  relationship  for 
the  division  of  the  Fourth,  at  least  in  artistically  developed  music.  But  in  the 
division  of  smaller  intervals  we  shall  find  it  applied  as  an  auxiliary  in  many  of  the 
less  usual  divisions  of  the  Greek  tetrachord  and  in  the  scales  of  Oriental  nations. 
But  arbitrary  divisions  which  are  independent  of  tonal  relationship,  disappeared 
^  everywhere  in  exact  proportion  to  the  higher  development  of  the  musical  art. 

We  will  now  inquire  what  kind  of  a  scale  we  should  obtain  by  pursuing  to  its 
consequences  the  natural  relationship  of  the  tones.  We  shall  consider  musical 
tones  to  be  related  in  the  first  degree  which  have  two  identical  partial  Umes  ;  and 
related  in  the  second  degree,  when  they  are  both  related  in  the  first  degree  to  some 
third  musical  tone.  The  louder  the  coincident  in  proportion  to  the  non-coincident 
partials  of  compound  tones  related  in  the  first  degree,  the  closer  is  their  relation- 
ship and  the  more  easily  will  both  singers  and  hearers  feel  the  common  character 
of  both  the  tones.  Hence  it  follows  that  the  feeling  for  tonal  relationship  ought  to 
differ  with  the  qualities  of  tone :  and  I  believe  that  this  states  a  &ct  in  nature, 
because  flutes  and  the  soft  stops  of  organs,  on  which  chords  are  somewhat  colour- 
less owing  to  an  absence  of  upper  partials  and  a  consequent  incomplete  definition 
of  dissonances,  retain  much  of  the  same  colourless  character  in  melodies.  This, 
I  think,  depends  upon  the  fact,  that,  for  such  qualities  of  tone,  the  recognition  of 
IT  the  natural  intervals  of  the  Thirds  and  Sixths,  and  perhaps  even  of  the  Fourths 
and  Fifths,  does  not  result  from  the  immediate  sensation  of  the  hearer,  but  at  most 
from  his  recollection.  When  he  knows  that  on  other  instruments  and  in  singing 
he  has  been  able  by  immediate  sensation  to  recognise  the  Thirds  and  Sixths  as 
naturally  related  tones,  he  acknowledges  them  as  well-known  intervals  even  when 
executed  by  a  flute  or  on  the  soft  stops  of  an  organ.  But  the  mere  recollection  of 
an  impression  cannot  possibly  have  the  same  freshness  and  power  as  the  immediate 
sensation  itself. 

Since  the  closeness  of  relationship  depends  on  the  loudness  of  the  coincident 
upper  partial  tones,  and  those  having  a  higher  ordinal  number  are  usually  weaker 
than  those  having  a  lower  one,  the  relationship  of  two  tones  is  generally  weaker, 
the  greater  the  ordinal  number  of  the  coincident  partials.  These  ordinal  numbers, 
as  the  reader  will  recollect  from  the  theory  of  consonant  intervals,  also  give  the 
ratio  of  the  vibrational  numbers  of  the  corresponding  notes. 
^  In  the  following  table,  the  first  horizontal  line  contains  the  ordinal  numbers  of 
the  partial  tones  of  the  tonic  o,  and  the  first  vertical  column  those  of  the  corre- 
sponding tone  in  the  scale.  Where  the  corresponding  vertical  columns  and  hori- 
zontal lines  intersect,  the  name  of  the  tone  of  the  scale  is  given  for  which  this 
coincidence  holds.  Only  such  notes  are  admitted  as  are  distant  from  the  tonic  by 
less  than  an  Octave.  Below  each  degree  of  the  scale  are  placed  the  two  ordinal 
numbers  of  the  coincident  partials,  which  will  serve  as  a  scale  for  measuring  the 
closeness  of  the  relationship. 

*   [Other  experimental  InstrameDts  will  be  not  so  harsh  but  quite  near  enough  to  shew  its 

described  in  App.  XX.  sect.  F.    The  Harmoni-  character.  For  the  intervals  used  by  violinists, 

cal  gives  only  the  just  major  Third  4  :  5.    Its  see  also  App.  XX.  sect.  G.  arts.  6  and  7. — 

nearest  approach  to  the  Pythagorean  64  :  81,  Translator. '\ 
or  408  cents,  is  'Bb  :  X>,  =  63  :*8o,  or  413  cents, 


Digitized  by 


Google 


CHAP.   XIV. 


PENTATONIC  SCALES. 


257 


Partial  Tones  of  the  Tonio 

X 

9 

3 

4 

5 

6 

c 
I :  I 

I  :  2 

C 

2 :  I 

2 :  2 

9 
2:3 

2:4 

F 
3:2 

c 
3:3 

/ 
3:4 

3:5 

3:6 

C 
4:2 

0 
4:3 

c 

4:4 

4:5 

4?6 

5:3 

ilb 
5:4 

c 
5:5 

6b 
s:6 

C 
6:3 

F 
6:4 

A 
6:5 

c 
6:6 

IF 


In  this  systematic  comparison  we  find  the  following  series  of  notes  lying  in 
the  octave  above  the  fandamental  note  c,  and  related  to  the  tonic  0  in  the  first 
degree,  arranged  in  the  order  of  their  relationship : 

c  c^  g  f  a  c  eb 

1:1         1:2        2:3        3:4        3:5        4:5 
and  the  following  series  in  the  descending  octave : 
C  F  O  E^} 


5:6 


0 

I :  I 


4:  3 


5  :3 


5:4 


A 
6:5 


The  series  is  discontinued  when  the  resultant  intervals  become  very  close. 
Intervals  adapted  for  practical  use  must  not  be  too  close  to  be  easily  taken  and  ^ 
distinguished.  What  is  the  smallest  interval  admissible  in  a  scale  is  a  question 
which  different  nations  have  answered  differently  according  to  the  different 
direction  of  their  taste,  and  perhaps  also  according  to  the  different  dehcacy  of 
their  ear. 

It  seems  that  in  the  first  stages  of  the  development  of  music  many  nations 
avoided  the  use  of  intervals  of  less  than  a  Tone,  and  hence  formed  scales,  which 
alternated  in  intervals  from  a  Tone  to  a  Tone  and  a  half.  According  to  examples 
collected  by  M.  F^tis,*  a  scale  of  this  kind  is  found  not  only  among  the  Chinese 
but  also  among  the  other  branches  of  the  Mongol  race,  among  the  Malays  of  Java 
and  Sumatra,  the  inhabitants  of  Hudson's  Bay,  the  Papuas  of  New  Guinea,  the 
inhabitants  of  New  Caledonia,  and  the  Fullah  negroes.f  The  five-stringed  lyre 
(Kissar)  of  the  inhabitants  of  North  Africa  and  Abyssinia,  which  is  represented  in 
the  bas-r^liefis  of  the  Assyrian  palaces  as  an  instrument  play  ed  on  by  captives,  was 
also,  according  to  Villoteai},^  tuned  by  the  scale  of  five  degrees  :  ^ 

^  — a  — 6  — d'  — e'§ 

Traces  of  an  ancient  scale  of  this  kind  are  clearly  furnished  by  the  five-stringed 
lyre  or  lute  {KiBdpa)  of  the  Greeks.  At  least  Terpander  (circa  B.C.  700-650),  who 
played  a  conspicuous  part  in  the  development  of  ancient  Greek  music,  and  who 
added  a  seventh  string  to  the  former  Cithara  of  six  strings,  used  a  scale  composed 
of  a  tetrachord  and  a  trichord,  having  the  compass  of  an  Octave  and  tuned 
thus : — 

e^f^g^a-'b^  —  d'  —  e'** 


*  Hiataire  GinSrale  de  la  Musique^  Paris, 
1869,  vol.  i. 

t  [See  App.  XX.  sect.  E.  for  pentatonio 
scales  in  Java,  China,  and  Japan. — Trans- 
latar.] 

X  DescripHons  des  Instrum&nts  de  Mtisique 
des  Orieniaux ;  chap.  xiii.  in  the  Description 


de  VEffypte.    6tat  Modeme. 

§  [This  is  probably  only  a  rude  approxi- 
mation or  a  guess.  See  App.  XX.  sect.  K.  for 
observations  on  existing  pentatonic  scales  actu- 
ally heard.— Translator.] 

**  Nicomachus  makes  Philolaus  say  (edit. 
Meibomii,  p.  17),  *  From  the  Hypate  Jte)  to  ' 


Digitized  by  VJ 


8^'§k 


258  PENTATONIC  SCALES.  part  ni. 

in  which  there  is  no  c',  and  the  upper  tetrachord  has  no  interval  of  a  Semitone, 
although  there  is  an  interval  of  this  kind  in  the  lower.* 

Olympos  (circa  B.C.  660-620),  who  introduced  Asiatic  flute  music  into  Greece 
aiid  ieidiBbpted  it  to  Greek  tastes,  transformed  the  Greek  Doric  scale  into  one  of  five 
tones,  the  old  enharmonic  scale 

b^^c e^^f at 

This  seems  to  indicate  that  he  brought  a  scale  of  five  tones  with  him  &om  Asia, 
and  merely  borrowed  the  use  of  the  intervals  of  a  Semitone  firom  the  Greeks. 
Among  the  more  cultivated  nations,  the  Chinese  and  the  Celts  of  Scotland  and 
Irelsiiid  still  retain  the  scale  of  five  notes  without  Semitones,  although  both  have 
also  become  acquainted  with  the  complete  scale  of  seven  notes. 

Among  the  Chinese,  a  certain  prince  Tsay-yu  is  said  to  have  introduced  the 

IF  scale  of  seven  notes  amid  great  opposition  firbm  conservative  musicians.  The 
division  of  the  Octave  into  twelve  Semitones,  and  the  transposition  of  scales  have 
also  been  discovered  by  this  intelligent  and  skilful  nation.  But  the  melodies  tran- 
scribed by  travellers  mostly  belong  to  the  scale  of  fi^e  notes.  The  Gaels  and  Erse 
have  likewise  become  acquainted  with  the  diatonic  scale  of  seven  tones  by  means 
of  psalmody,  and  in  the  present  form  of  their  popular  melodies  the  missing  tones 
are  sometimes  just  touched  as  appoggiature  or  passing  notes.  These  are,  however, 
in  many  cases  merely  modern  improvements,  as  may  be  seen  on  comparing  the 
older  forms  of  the  melodies,  and  it  is  usually  possible  to  omit  the  notes  which  do 
not  belong  to  the  scale  of  five  tones  without  impairing  the  melody.  This  is  not 
only  true  of  the  older  melodies,  but  of  more  modem  popular  airs  which  were  com- 
posed during  the  last  two  centuries,  whether  by  learned  or  unlearned  musicians. 
Hence  the  Gaels  as  well  as  the  Chinese,  notwithstanding  their  acquaintance  with 
the  modem  tonal  system,  hold  fast  by  the  old.t    And  it  cannot  be  denied  that  by 

if  avoiding  the  Semitones  of  the  diatonic  scale,  Scotch  airs  receive  a  peculiarly  bright 
and  mobile  character,  although  we  cannot  say  as  much  for  the  Chinese.  Both 
Gaels  and  Chinese  make  up  for  the  small  number  of  tones  within  the  Octave  by 
great  compass  of  voice. § 

The  scale  of  five  tones  admits  of  a  certain  variety  in  its  construction.  Assume 
c  as  the  tonic  and  add  to  it  the  nearest  related  notes  in  the  ascending  Octave,  till 
you  come  to  a  Semitone.    This  gives 

c  —  c'  —  g — / — a. 

The  next  note  e  would  form  a  Semitone  with/.  In  the  descending  Octave  we  find 
in  the  same  way 

c—-C  —  F—-G  —  Ei). 

The  great  gaps  in  the  scales  between  c  and/  in  the  first,  and  between  G  and  c 

H  in  the  second  are  filled  up  by  tones  related  in  the  second  degree.     Since  the  tones 

related  to  the  Octave  can  only  be  repetitions  of  those  directly  related  to  the  tonic, 

Mese  (a)  wag  a  Fourth,  from  the  Mese  (a)  to  bition  in  London,  1884,  gives  many  varieties 

the  Ncte  (e')  a  Fifth,  from  the  Nete  {e')  to  the  of  this  scale,  see  App.  XX.  sect.  E.  Japan. — 

Trite  (6)  a  Fourth,  from  the  Trite  (6)  to  the  Translator,] 

Hypate  (e)  a  Fifth.*    This  shews  that  c,  not  6,  %  Chinese  Melodies,  in  Ambrosch's    Oe- 

was  the  missing  note.  schichte  der  Muaikt  vol.  i.  pp.  30,  34,  35.     Of 

*  [The  upper  tetrachord  was  thus  reduced  Scotch    melodies  there   is   a   fine  collection 

to  a  trichord,  while  the  lower  remained  a  per-  with  reference  to  the  authorities  and  the  older 

feet  tetrachord.    If  we  take  Pythagorean  into-  forms  in  G.  F.  Graham's  Songs  of  Scotland, 

nation  the  cents  are  e  90  /  204  g  204  a  204  3  vols.  Edinburgh,  1859.    The  modem  piano- 

b  294  d'  204  e'. — TranslatorJ]  forte  accompaniment  which  has  been  added,  is 

f  [Taking    Pythagorean    intonation,    the  often  ill  enough  suited  to  the  character  of  the 

cents  in  the  intervals  are  6  90  c  408  egof  408  airs. 

a  204  b.    The  account  of  the  popular  tuning  §   [Exclusive  of  the  two  drones  there  are 

of  the  Eo-to,  the  national  Japanese  instru-  only  9  tones  on  the  bagpipe.    For  the  whole 

ment,  furnished  by  the  Japanese,  but  in  Euro-  of  these  observations  see  App.  XX.  sect.  K. — 

pean  notes,  at  the  International  Health  Ezhi-  Translator.] 


Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


CHAP.  XIV. 


PENTATONIC  SCALES. 


259 


the  next  tones  to  be  considered  are  those  related  to  the  upper  Fifth  g,  and  lower 
Fifth  F,  and  these  are  d  (the  Fifth  above  the  upper  Fifth  g)  and  B^  (the  Fifth 
•below  the  lower  iFiith  F).    We  thus  obtain  the  scales* 


i)  Ascending 

c  —  d  —  ^-f — g — a  —  ^c' 

If          $     t      * 

2)  Descending 

C  —  ^E^—F—O  —  ^B0—c 

*       J       t           V      2 

But  in  place  of  the  tones  more  distantly  related  to  the  tonic  in  the  first  degree, 
both  systems  of  tones  related  in  the  second  degree  might  be  used,  and  this  would 
give  a  scale  resulting  from  a  simple  progression  by  Fifths,  as 


3) 


c  —  d  —  -*-/ — g  —  -^ftt> — cf 


Then  there  are  also  some  more  irregular  forms  of  this  scale  of  five  tones,  in 
which  the  major  Third  e  replaces  the  Fourth  /,  which  is  more  nearly  related  to 
the  tonic  c.  This  transformation  is  probably  due  to  the  modem  preference  for  the 
major  mode,  and  it  has  made  its  appearance  in  very  many  Scotch  melodies.  The 
scale  is  then 

4)  c  —  d  —  e  —  ^-^g  —  a — ^^c' 


f 


The  examples  of  a  similar  exchange  of  the  Fifth  g  for  the  minor  Sixth  at>  are 
doubtful.    This  would  give  the  scale 

s)  0— ^JSb— i?"—- '4)— jBi>— c 

The  scale    6)  c — — 4> — / — 9 — «  —  ~^c 

f    *   i    4       »  ^ 

in  which  all  the  notes  are  related  in  the  first  degree,  but  for  which  the  nearest 
notes  to  the  tonic,  either  way,  are  a  Tone  and  a  Semitone  distant  from  it,  has  not 
jet  been  discovered  in  actual  use. 

The  above  five  forms  of  the  scale  of  five  tones  can  all  be  so  transposed  that 
they  can  be  played  on  the  black  notes  of  a  piano  without  touching  the  white  ones.f 
•This  is  the  well-known  simple  rule  for  composing  Scotch  melodies.J    Any  one  of 

4)  c  204  d  182  e,  316  g  182  a,  316  c'. 

5)  C3i6^'bi82ii^3i6-4»b  204B»bi82c. 

6)  c  316  g'b  182/  204  g  182  a,  316  c— 
Translator.] 

■f  [In  the  following  way— the  numbers  re- 
ferring to  the  BohemeB  in  this  page,  and  the 
oorresponding  cents,  of  course,  belonging  to 
equal  temperament : 

ZOO  eft 

300  eft  200  d't 

300  c't  200  d't  300  ft  200  g't 

300  eft  200  d't  300/15 

Anderson's  College,  Glasgow,  in  The  Thistle, 
*  a  miscellany  of  Scottish  song,  with  notes 
critical  and  historical ;  the  melodies  arranged 
in  their  natural  modes ;  with  an  introduction, 
explaining  the  construction  and  characteristics 
of  Scottish  music,  the  Principles,  Laws,  and 
Origin  of  Melody  *  (Glasgow,  Bee.  1883),  says, 
p.  viii. :  *  The  pentatonic  form  of  the  scale  is 
used  in  Scotland,  but  not  to  a  greater  extent 
than  in  the  national  music  of  some  other 
countries.  A  general  idea  seems  to  prevail 
that  Scottish  music  can  be  played  upon  the 
five  black  digitals  of  the  pianoforte ,  re|j|e^ej^ing 


*  [In  the  following  investigation  the 
Author  all  along  assumes  harmonic  forms  of 
•the  intervals,  which  are  certainly  modem. 
The  cents  in  the  five  forms  cited,  as  deter- 
mined from  the  ratios  given,  are  : 

i)  c  204  d  294  /  204  g  204  a  316  c'. 

2)  C3i6^*b  182^204  0294  Bb  204c. 

3)  c  204  (Z  294/  204  gf  294  6  b  204  c'. 

1)  cj  200  d»  300/5  200  g»  200  aJJ 

2)  dt  300  ft  300  gt  200  at 

3)  gt  200  at 

4)  ^  200  ^«  200  at 

5)  At  300  ct  200  dt  300/1 200  gt  200  at 

And  this  shews  that  all  five  are  formed  by  a 
Bimi^e  succession  of  tempered  Fifths,  for  the 
five  black  notes  arranged  in  order  of  Fifths  are 
ft  700  et  700  gt  700  dt  700  at .  The  piano 
being  tuned  in  equal  temperament  gives  very 
nearly  perfect  Fifths,  and  hence  very  well 
imitates  a  succession  of  five  notes  thus  tuned. 
If,  however,  the  Fifths  are  perfect,  then  every 
200  and  300  cents  in  the  above  scheme  becomes 
204  and  294,  differences  which  few  ears  will 
perceive  in  melody. 

%  [Mr.  Colin  Brown,  Euing  Lecturer  on 
the  Science,  Theory,  and  History  of  Music, 


26o 


PENTATONIC  SCALES. 


PART  nr. 


the  five  black  notes  may  then  be  used  as  a  tonic,  bat  the  ^  or  Ajj^  having  no  Fifih 
(F  or  Eijf)  among  the  black  notes  has  a  very  doubtful  effect  as  a  tonic. 

The  following  are  examples  of  the  use  of  these  various  scales  of  five  tones  :— 
The  First  Scale  without  Third  or  Seventh.    Chinese,  after  John  Barrow.* 


I. 


» 


-#-^- 


^^m 


^m 


;st=s^ 


±r- 


i 


m 


wt: 


df    ij- 


^ 


at^ 


■mzM 


4-- 


m 


i 


^^^f=^ 


it:=it 


:#tz«b;i* 


"CT- 


2.  To  the  Second  Scale,  without  Second  or  Sixth,  belong  most  Scotch  airs 
which  have  a  minor  character.  In  the  modem  forms  of  these  airs  one  or  other  of 
the  missing  tones  is  often  transiently  touched.  Here  follows  an  older  form  of  the 
air  called  Cockle  Shells  :t 


^^^ 


3^^^ 


-wj- 


W       0      ^  '- '— ^ # 


1! 


what  is  popularly  known  as  the  Caledonian 
scale ;  but  any  one  who  will  take  the  trouble 
to  examine  Scottish  music  will  find  that  not 
more  than  a  twentieth  part  of  our  old  melodies 
are  pentatonic,  or  constructed  upon  this  form 
of  the  scale.  In  Dauney's  work,  where  the 
Skene  MSS.  (the  oldest  collection  extant)  are 
noted,  this  statement  is  fully  verified/  I  have 
examined  the  first  36  airs  as  printed  in  The 
Thistle^  and  I  found  only  one  which  was 
strictly  pentatonic,  p.  5i»  No.  8,  Lament  for 
Huaridh  Mar,  Macleod  of  Macleod— Dun- 
vegan  1626.  But  in  nearly  a  quarter  of  the  airs 
the  Semitones  were  introduced  by  an  unac- 
cented note  which  looked  to  be  modem,  as  in 
Jioxfs  Wife,  p.  10,  and  the  Banks  and  Braes 
o'  Bonnie  Boon,  p.  48,  on  the  last  of  which 
Mr.  Brown  observes,  p.  49  =  *  With  pentatonic 
theorists  Ye  Banks  and  Braes  is  a  favourite 
example  of  this  assumed  peculiarity  of  Scottish 
music.  But  it  can  only  be  brought  into  the 
pentatonic  scale  by  being  played  in  an  incom- 
plete form-.'  The  only  places  in  which  the 
Seventh  gt  occurs  are  the  cadence  e'/5  9^  «' 
(which  occurs  twice,  and  is  evidently  out  of 
character,  and  should  be  e'  fl  a'  o'),  and  the 
flourished  ad  libitum  cadence /'«  e"  d"  c"5  h" 
containing  the  Fourth  d,  (which  should  clearly 
be  f'%  e"  c"t  b').  And  many  of  the  others 
can  be  probably  '  restored '  in  a  similar  fashion. 


Thus  of  Bxry's  Wife  Mr.  Brown  himself  aays, 
p.  1 1,  *  played  as  a  dance  tune  it  is  pentatonic,' 
and  gives  the  substitutes  for  his  version,  which 
are  clearly  the  more  ancient  forms.  Mr.  Brown 
gives  as  the  marks  of  Scotch  music  (pp.  ix.,  x.) 
I.  its  modal  character,  being  constructed  on 
the  ancient  seven  modes ;  2.  its  modtdatton  or 
change  of  mode,  which  is  constant ;  3.  almost 
absence  of  transition  or  change  of  key;  4. 
preponderance  of  minor  forms  of  the  scale ; 
5.  almost  absence  of  sharp  Sevenths  in  the 
minors  \  6.  cadences  on  to  every  note  of  the 
scale,  and  double  cadences  closing  on  an  unac- 
cented note,  which  are  simple  (repeating  the 
cadential  tone)  or  compound  (the  unaccented 
tone  differing  from  the  preceding). —  2Vaiu- 
lator.] 

*  [Scale,  tempered  d  200  e  300  g  200  a  200 
b  300,  d\  That  is,  no  /B  and  no  c» .  All 
these  scales  are  merely  the  best  representa- 
tives in  European  notation  of  the  sensations 
produced  by  the  scales  on  European  listeners. 
They  cannot  be  received  as  correct  represen- 
tations of  the  notes  actually  played. — Trans- 
lator,] 

t  Playford's  Dancing  Master,  ed.  1721, 
The  first  edition  appeared  in  1657. — Songs  of 
Scotland,  vol.  iii.  p.  170.  [Scale,  d  300  /  200 
^  200  a  300  c'  200  d',  without  e  or  b\>.— Trans- 
lator.] 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


CHAP.  XIV. 


PENTATONIC  SCALES. 


261 


3.  F<yr  the  Third  Scale,  without  Third  and  Sixth, 
bagpipe  tune.* 


Gaelic.    Probably  an  old 


^ 


± 


^ 


^^ 


g^^ 


:5tEr*t 


P 


Blythe,blytheand    mer-ry    are  we, 
Gan-ty  days  we've  of  -  ten  seen  ;  A 


Blythe  are    we, 
night  like  this  we 


one  and 
ne  -  ver 


^ 


^feg 


rbe 


«=»c 


^ 


*=^^ 


¥ 


The  gloam-ing  saw  us    all    sit  down,  And  mei-kle  mirth  has  been  our  fa'. 


P 


Then 
D.C. 


b^JLj^zfe 


^S 


^ 


let  the  toast  and  sang   go  round.  Till   chan  -  ti  -  oleer  be  -  gins      to   craw : 

4.  To  the  Fourth  Scale,  without  Fourth  or  Seventh,  belong  most  Scotch  airs 
which  have  the  character  of  a  major  mode.  Since  dozens  of  Scotch  tunes  of  this 
kind  are  to  be  found  in  every  collection,  and  are  perfectly  well  known,  T  give  here 
a  Chinese  temple  hymn,  after  6it8churin,t  as  an  example : 


H 


T 


^ 


I 


b    I   r? 


?2I 


=t 


S^ 


=^2= 


e^^g=f 


2^ 


zcz: 


a^ 


22= 


S: 


rt 


g 


-p2: 


z^ 


5.  For  the  Fifth  Scale,  without  Second  and  Fifth,  I  have  found  no  perfectly 
pure  examples.  But  there  are  melodies  with  either  only  the  Fifth  or  else  with  a  IT 
mere  transient  use  of  both  Second  and  Fifth.  In  the  latter  case  the  minor  Second 
is  used,  giving  it  the  character  of  the  ecclesiastical  Phrygian  tone,  for  example  in 
the  very  beautiful  air,  Auld  Bohin  Gray.  I  give  an  example  with  the  tonic  J%^ 
in  which  the  Second  (^  or  g)  is  altogether  absent,  and  the  fifth  c^  is  only  once 
transiently  touched,  so  that  it  might  just  as  well  have  been  omitted. 


JM^^s^^ 


l—^^\  ^    J^ 


Will 
Let 


ye 

UB 


go» 


las  -  sie. 


go 


To      the    braes 


Bal 


i 


s^ 


w 


-    quhid  -  der,     Where 


the 


blae    -     her    - 


ries     grow,       Mang      the 


^ 


^ 


5: 


=F 


bon    -   nie  bloom  -  in     hea  -  ther ;     Where    the        deer       and       the 

Sport     the        lang        aim  -  mer 


^^ 


-j^  J 1  J 


^ 


rae, 
day, 


Light 
On 


the 


bound 
braes 


inff 
o 


the     '     ge    -  ther, 
Bal      -  quhid  -der? 


*  There  is  a  Chinese  tone  of  the  same  kind 
in  Ambrosch,  Zoc.  cU,  vol.  i.  p.  34,  second  piece. 
Another,  with  a  single  occnrrence  of  the  Sixth, 
My  Peggie  is  a  young  thing,  may  be  seen  in 
Songs  of  Scotland^  vol.  iii.  p.  10.  [Scale, 
€  200  /g  300  a  200  6  300  d'  200  e\  without  g 
or  c.    On  the  bagpipe,  see  App.  XX.  sect.  £. 


Probably  the  scale  of  the  bagpipe  has  been 
unaltered  since  its  importation  from  the  East, 
and  it  probably  never  could  have  played  such 
a  scale  as  it  is  here  supposed  capable  of  per- 
forming.—TnansZator.] 

t  Ambrosch,  loc.  cit.  vol.  i.  p.  30.    To  the 
same  class  belongs  the  first  piece  on  p.  35  after 


Digitized  by  VjOOQlC 


262  TETEACHOBDS.  pabt  iix- 

We  might  also  in  this  example  assume  b  as  the  tonic»  and  regard  the  conclusions 
as  formed  upon  the  dominant  and  subdominant  in  the  old-fashioned  way.*  In 
these  scales  of  five  tones  the  determination  of  the  tonic  is  much  more  doubtful 
than  in  the  scales  of  seven  tones. 

The  rule  usually  given  for  the  G-aeHc  and  Chinese  scales,  to  omit  the  Fourth 
and  Seventh,  applies  therefore  only  to  the  fourth  of  the  above  scales,  which  cor- 
responds to  our  major  scale.  True  this  scale  is  often  used  in  the  usual  Scotcli 
airs  of  the  present  day,  and  is  probably  due  to  the  reaction  of  our  modem  tonal 
system.  But  the  examples  here  adduced  shew  that  every  possible  position  may  be 
assumed  by  the  tonic  in  the  scale  of  five  tones,  if  indeed  we  allow  these  scales  to 
have  a  tonic  at  all.  In  Scotch  melodies  the  omissions  in  both  major  and  minor 
scales  are  so  contrived  as  to  avoid  the  intervals  of  a  Semitone,  and  substitute  fior 
them  intervals  of  a  Tone  and  a  half.  Among  the  Chinese  airs,  however,  I  have 
%  found  one  which  belongs  rather  to  the  old  Greek  enharmonic  system,  to  be  con- 
sidered presently,  and  it  will  be  explained  at  the  same  time  (p.  265c). 

We  now  proceed  to  the  construction  of  scales  with  seven  degrees.  The  first 
form  was  developed  in  Greece  under  the  influence  of  the  tetrachordal  divisions. 
The  ancient  Greek  melodies  had  a  small  compass  and  few  degrees,  a  peculiarity 
especially  emphasised  even  by  later  authors,  as  Plutarch,  but  it  is  also  found  among 
most  nations  in  the  early  stages  of  their  musical  cultivation.  Hence  the  scale  was 
at  first  formed  within  a  less  compass  than  an  Octave,  namely  within  the  tetrachord. 
On  looking  within  this  compass  for  the  tones  nearest  related  to  the  limiting  tonic 
(/AcoTTy),  we  find  only  the  Thirds.  Thus  if  we  assume  e  (the  last  tone  in  the  tetra- 
chord, b — e)  as  a  tonic,  its  next  related  tone  within  the  compass  of  that  tetrachord 
is  c,  the  major  Third  below  e.    This  gives  : — 

1.  The  afunent  enharmonic  tetrachord  of  Olympos — 

b^^  c e 

^  i    i        ^ 

Archytas  was  the  first  to  settle  that  the  tuning  of  c  :  e  must  be  4  :  5  in  the 
enharmonic  mode.  The  next  most  closely  related  tone  to  e  would  be  the  minor 
Third  below  it.    Adding  this  we  obtain  : 

2.  The  older  chromatic  tetracJiord  of  the  Greeks — 

5  w  c  ^'  cjf  —  ^-^a 
I      f       J 

The  method  of  tuning  the  intervals  here  assigned  agrees  with  the  data  of 
Eratosthenes  (in  the  third  century  before  Christ).  The  interval  between  c  and  cjf 
in  this  case  corresponds  to  the  small  ratio  ^  [=  70  cents],  which  is  less  than  the 
Semitone  |f  [=  112  cents].  Next  to  it  comes  the  much  wider  interval,  cjf — e, 
corresponding  to  a  minor  Third.  We  should  obtain  a  more  even  distribution  of 
%  intervals,  by  measuring  the  minor  Third  upwards  from  the  lowest  tone  of  the 
tetrachord.     This  gives  rise  to 

3.  The  diatonic  tetrachord — 

few  c  —  d  —  e 

This  is  the  tuning  assigned  by  Ptolemy  for  the  diatonic  tetrachord.    Here  we 

Bftrrow  and  Amiot.     fSoale,/  200  g  200  a  300  to  tonic,  dominant  and  subdominant,  implies 

c'  200  i'  3C»  /,  without  6  b  or  e.— Translator,]  harmonic  aoales,  which  pentatonio  scales  could 

*   [Taking /5  as  the  Tonic,  the  scale  would  not  have  been  originally.    Mr.  C.  Brown  gives 

be  No.  5,  without  Second  and  Fifth,  thus :  this  air  (Thistle,  p.  198)  as  here  printed,  but 

/5  300  a  200  6  300  d  200  «  200 /»  ?g;8  it  varies  between  hifl  modes  of  the  ^ 

.  ,  *       .        .,.    X     .    XI-         1  1^  k«  (Greek  Done,  Ecclesiastical  Phrygian)  and  5th 

but  taking  b  as  the  tonic  the  scale  would  be  J^   ^^^   ^^^^  ^^^   ^^^^^  E^l    Mixolydian). 

No.  2,  without  Second  and  Sixth,  as  fj^^  spelling  of  the  words  has  been  corrected 

6  300  d  200  «  200/5  300  a  200  6  by  his  edition.— Trntwiator.] 


which  is  altogether  different,    -^ny  referenoe 


Digitized  by 


Google 


CHAP.  XIV.  TETEACHORDS.  263 

must  observe  that  if  e  contmued  to  be  regarded  as  the  tonic,  d  would  hi^ve  only  a 
distant  relation  with  it  in  the  second  degree  through  the  auxilia2;y  tone  b,  J£  two 
tetrachords  had  been  connected,  as  was  very  oarly  done,  thus  : 

b e a 

a  closer  connection  in  the  second  degree  between  d  and  e  might  have  been  obtained 
by  tuning  (2  as  a  fifth  below  a.  Taking  g  as  i,  a  will  be  f ,  and  the  Fifth  below  it 
is  d  =  |.     We  thus  obtain  the  tetrachord 


b  -^  c  —  d  —  ( 


^    i 


which  agrees  with  the  tuning  assigned  by  Didymus  (in  the  first  century  before 
Christ).  % 

According  to  the  old  theory  of  Pythagoras,  which  will  be  examined  presently, 
all  the  intervals  of  the  diatonic  scale  should  be  tuned  by  means  of  intervals  of  a 
Fifth,  giving : 

5.  b   ^-   c  —  d  —  e 

*         -Si      «         I 


9 


The  tetrachord  thus  obtained  is  the  Greek  Doric,  which  is  considered  as  normal, 
and  made  the  basis  of  all  considerations  on  other  scales.  Accordingly  those  tones 
which  formed  the  lower  notes  of  the  semitonic  intervals  of  the  scale,  were,  at  least 
theoretically,  considered  as  the  immovable  limiting  tones  of  the  tetrachord  while 
the  intermediate  tones  might  change  their  position.  Practically  the  intonation  of 
even  these  fixed  tones  was  a  Uttle  changed,  as  Plutarch  tells  us,  which  may  mean 
that  in  the  Lydian,  and  Phrygian  modes,  &c.,  the  tonic  is  not  selected  from  one  of  IF 
these  so-called  fixed  tones  of  the  tetrachords.  Thus  we  shall  see  further  on,  that 
when  d  is  the  tonic,  the  b  in  the  natural  intonation  of  such  a  scale  does  not  form 
a  perfect  Fifth  with  e. 

The  tetrachord  could,  however,  be  differently  completed  by  inserting  tones  which 
formed  major  or  minor  Thirds  with  either  of  the  extreme  tones. 

Two  minor  Thirds  give  the  Phrygian  tetrachord — 

6.  d  —  e  ^^  f  —  g 

If  a  major  Third  were  taken  upwards  from  the  lower  extreme  tone,  and  a  minor 
Third  downwards  from  the  upper  extreme,  we  should  obtain  the  Lydian  tetrachord — 

_d  —  e  — / 


3 


i    n    I 


IT 

8.  Two  major  Thirds,  as  in  b  ^-  c  —  cqf— '  e,  would  form  a  variety  of  the 
chromatic  scale,  which  does  not  seem  to  have  been  used,  or  at  any  rate  not  to 
have  been  distinguished  from  the  chromatic  form.* 

*  [Adopting  the  notation  explained  later  on  may  be  seen,  and  the  oorreotness  of  the  trans- 

in  this  chapter,  these  tetrachords  may  be  ac-  positions  verified, 

curately  written  as  follows ;  Nos.  i,  3,  4  and  7  i.  Olympos    .        .    6, 112  c' 3866,' 

may  be  played  as  they  stand  on  the  Harmonioal,  2.  Old  Chromatic  .  6, 1 1 2  c'  70  c/t  3160/ 

and  Nos.  2,  6,  8  by  transposition  as  shewn  (play        .  a  iiza^iyoai^iOcf) 

below,  but  No.  5  requires  the  six  notes  forming  3.  Diatonic    .        .  S,  1 12  c'  204  d^  182  e/ 

5  perfect  Fifths,  and  these  do  not  occur  on  the  4.  Didymus    .        .  6, 1 12  (/ 182  (2/  2046/ 

Uarmonioal,  but  can  be  played  sufficiently  well  5.  Doric         .        .    6  90  (/  204  d'  204  e' 

on  any  tempered  harmonium.    Between  the  (not  playable  on  the  Harmonical) 

names  of  the  notes  are  inserted  the  number  of  6.  Phrygian    .        .  d  182  e,  134/*  182  ^ 

cents  in  the  interval  between  them.    By  re-  (play        .  ^  182  a,  134  6*  b  182  c') 

f erring  to  the  table  called  the  Duodenarium,  7.  Lydian       .        .  c  1S2  di  204  e,  112/ 

App.  XX.  sect.  E.  art.  18,  which  employs  the  8.  Unused      .        .  6,  1 12  c' 274  ^,'9  112  e/ 

same  notation,  the  exact  position  of  the  notes  (play        .  ^  1 12  a'b  274  6,  112  c') 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


264  TETRACHOEDS.  pabt  m. 

These  are  all  the  normal  snbdivisioiiB  of  the  tetraohord  that  have  been  used* 
But  other  subdivisions  occur  which  the  Greeks  themselves  termed  irrational  (SXoya)  ,* 
and  we  do  not  know  with  certainty  -how  for  they  were  practically  used.  One  of 
them,  the  soft  diatonic  mode,  makes  use  of  the  interval  6:7,  which  is  at  any  rate 
very  near  to  a  natural  consonance,  being  that  between  the  Fifth  and  the  subminor 
Seventh  of  the  fundamental  note,  an  interval  occasionally  used  in  harmonic  musio 
when  unaccompanied  singers  take  the  minor  Seventh  of  the  chord  of  the  dominant 
Seventh.    The  intervals  j  are  : 

9-  3JJ 

V r 

6:7 

By  lowering  the  Lichanos  the  Parhypate  is  also  flattened.    However,  the  small 
f  interval  ||  is  very  nearly  the  Pythagorean  Semitone,  which  expressed  approxi- 
mately is  f  ^. 

The  equal  diatonic  mode  of  Ptolemy,  which  was  divided  thus :  t 

10.  3:4 


contained  a  perfect  minor  Third  divided  as  evenly  as  possibla 

There  is  a  similar  succession  of  tones,  in  an  inverse  order,  in  the  modem 
Arabic  scale  as  measured  by  the  Syrian,  Michael  Meshaqah.§  In  this  case  the 
Octave  is  divided  into  twenty-four  Quartertones  ** ;  the  tetrachord  10  has  ten  of 
them,  its  lowest  interval  four,  and  each  of  the  upper  intervals  three.  Under  these 
circumstances  the  two  upper  intervals  together  form  very  nearly  a  minor  Third, 
^  which,  as  in  the  equal  diatonic  scale  of  the  Greeks,  is  divided  into  two  equal 
intervals,  without  paying  regard  to  any  sensible  relationship  of  the  intermediate 
tone  thus  produced. 

The  closer  the  interval,  the  more  easy  and  certain  is  its  division  into  two 
intervals,  by  the  mere  feeling  for  difference  of  pitch.  This  is,  in  particular,  poe- 
Bible  for  intervals  which  approach  to  the  limits  at  which  dififerences  of  pitch  are 

If  the  minor  Thirds  df  and  e  g  were  taken  as  monioal,  as  d'"  c/"  "/"  9^"»  downwards  as  g**' 

Pythagorean «  294  cents,  tetrachord  6  would  ^^f"e^"'d"\    The  division  of  the  minor  Hiird 

become  d  204  e  90  /  204  g^  which  is  more  in-  g  :  e|  =  3i6  cents  into  151  and  165  cents  is  of 

telligible.  course  only  approximative.    But  it  is  a  purely 

On  referring    to    App.  XX.  sect.  D.  the  natural  tetrachord  of  which  ^204/ 112  e,  182  d( 

ratios  corresponding  to  each  of  these  numbers  is  a  deformation. — Translator.'] 

of  cents  will  be  found. — Translator.']  §  Journal  of  the  American  Oriental  Sodeiy^ 

*  [That  is,  strictly,  having  a  ratio  not  ex-  vol.  i.  p.  173,  1847. 

pressible  by  whole  numbers.— Tronstotor.]  *♦  [If  the  Octave  is  divided  equally  into  24 

f  [The  notes  which  would  form  tetrachord  9  quarters,  each  of  which  is  half  an  equal  Semi- 

m  might  be  written  in  the  Translator's  notation,  tone  or  50  cents,  we  can  write  it  by  using  the 

descending  from  left  to  right,  additional  sign  q  (a  turned  b,  standing  for  q 

'db  85  a,  182  g  231  '/.  ^6  initial  of  quarter)  to  represent  an  added 

The  three  first  notes  could  be  played  on  the  Q^arteriione,  5  being  two  Quartertones.   and 

Harmonical.    The  interval  23ilBents  could  be  «*»  ^^^  Quartertones    thus  ascendmg  c  c<K 

played  on  it  downwards  as  C  231  '6b,  but  the  ^« <; «*»  ^  ^^ descending^  dM d\>c^c, usmg db 

whole  tetrachord  cannot  be  played  on  it.    Here  »«  the  equiviUent  of  c«  .    Then  the  principal 

85  cents  represent  21  :  20,  while  the  Pytha-  »<^»^«  <>*  Meshaqah  (see  App.  XX.  sect.  K.)  is 

gorean  Semitone  256  .*  243  is  90  cents.    The  a  2006  150  c'q  150  (2'  200  e'  150/q  150  ^  200  a'. 

di^rence  Ib  smaU  but  perceptible- rmn.-  ^^^^  ^^^  tetrachord  «  :  d',  which  represent. 

t  [Using  the  Dotation  ■■/  for  the  i  ith  har-  ^°^  *«*«»*.  ^  °"«  ^^^".'^  «'  «»  f°*«  »' 

monic  of  C.I0  that "  is  equivalent  to  33  :  3*  or  4  Quirtertones.  and  two  of   150  cents  or  3 

53  cents,  tetrachord  loTay  be  written  down-  Q^rtertones.    This  mterval  of  3  Quartertonea 

wards-  representsthe  trumpetmtervals*'/ I  gf*  II  :  12 

a  lu  'Y  i6«;  e   182  d  ""  '5i   cents,  and  c  :  »'/- 10  :  11 « 165  cents, 

y    ->     J      ^   ^  '  and  was  introduced  into  Arabia  by  the  luti&t 

This  is  simply,  in  order,  the  12,  11,  10,  and  9th  Zalzal,  who  died  about  1000  years  ago,  and  ia 

harmonic  of  c,  and  can  be  played  on  the  horn  much  used  in  the  East. — Translator^ 
or  trumpet,  and  on  the  5th  octave  of  the  Har- 


Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


CHAP.  XIV.  TETRACHORDS,  265 

distinguishable  by  the  ear.  The  distinctness  with  which  the  yet  sensible  difference 
can  be  felt  then  famishes  a  measure  of  its  magnitude.  In  this  sense  we  have 
probably  to  explain  the  possibility  of  the  later  enharmonic  mode  of  the  Greeks, 
which,  however,  had  already  feillen  into  disuse  in  the  time  of  Aristoxenus,  and  was 
perhaps  hunted  up  again  by  later  writers  as  an  antiquarian  curiosity.  In  this 
mode  the  Semitone  of  the  ancient  enharmonic  mode  already  mentioned  (No.  i , 
p.  2626)  was  again  subdivided  into  two  Quartertones,  so  that  a  tetrachord  was 
produced  like  the  chromatic  one,  but  with  closer  intervals  between  the  adjacent 
tones.    The  division  of  this  enharmonic  tetrachord  *  was 

II.  3:4 


n  U  i 

This  Quartertone  can  only  be  considered  as  a  transition  in  the  melodic  move- 
ment towards  the  lowest  extreme  of  the  tetrachord.  A  similar  interval  occurs  in  H 
this  way  in  existing  Oriental  music.  A  distinguished  musician  whom  I  requested 
to  pay  attention  to  it  on  a  visit  to  Cairo,  wrote  to  me  as  follows  :  '  This  evening 
I  have  been  listening  attentively  to  the  song  on  the  minarets,  to  try  to  appreciate 
the  Quartertones,  which  I  had  not  supposed  to  exist,  as  I  had  thought  that  the 
Arabs  sang  mit  of  tune.  But  to-day  as  I  was  with  the  dervishes  I  became  certain 
that  such  Quartertones  existed,  and  for  the  following  reasons.  Many  passages 
in  litanies  of  this  kind  end  with  a  tone  which  was  at  first  the  Quartertone  and 
then  ended  in  the  pure  tone.f  As  the  passage  was  frequently  repeated,  I  was  able 
to  observe  this  every  time,  and  I  found  the  intonation  invariable.*  The  Greek 
writers  on  music  themselves  say  that  it  is  difficult  to  distinguish  the  enharmonic 
Quartertones.^ 

The  later  interpreters  of  Greek  musical  theory  have  mostly  advanced  the 
opinion  that  the  above-mentioned  differences,  which  the  Greeks  called  colourings 
(xpoat),  were  merely  speculative  and  never  came  into  practical  use.§  They  con-  ^ 
sider  that  these  distinctions  were  too  delicate  to  produce  any  esthetic  effect  except 
on  an  incredibly  well  cultivated  ear.  But  it  seems  to  me  that  this  opinion  could 
never  have  been  entertained  or  advanced  by  modem  theorists,  if  any  of  them  had 
practically  attempted  to  form  these  various  tonal  modes  and  to  compare  them  by 
ear.     On  an  harmonium  which  will  shortly  be  described  I  am  able  to  compare 

*  [It  is  not   to  be  supposed   that   these  nnmber  of  vibrations  for  the  sharpened  and 

two  Quartertones,  differing  only  by  two  cents  normal  note,  which  gave  the  interval  as  48 

(32  :  31=55    cents,  31  :  30  =  57   cents),  were  cents.    The  effect  was  very  peculiar,  but  can 

exactly  produced.    The  lutist  or  lyrist  would  of  course  be  easily  imitated  on  the  violin.    On 

tune  his  Fourth  c  : /by  ear  (tolerably  correctly),  the  classical  Indutn  instrument,  the  Vina,  the 

then  a  major  Third  below  f  or  d^\>  also  by  ear  frets  are  very  high,  sometimes  about  an  inch, 

(and  probably  very  incorrectly  on  account  of  Hence  by  pressing  down  the  string  behind  the 

the  great  difficulty  of  tuning  a  major  Third),  fret,  the  tension  could  be  greatly  increased,  and 

and  then  would  by  feeling  divide  the  remaining  as  much  as  a  Semitone  could  be  easily  added, 

interval  in  halves  as  well  as  he  could.     Using  so  that  the  scale  could  be  indefinitely  altered 

c  :  c<\   for  the    approximate    Quartertone  he  without  changing  the  frets,  which  were  fixed  ^ 

would  have  a&ou^c  56  cq  56  <2'b  386/,  or  some-  with  wax.     On  the  Arabic  Babab  and   the 

thing  sufficiently  like  it.     Meshaqah's  c   50  curious  Chinese  fiddles,  which  have  no  frets  or 

cq  50  cS 400 /would  doubtless  have  been  near  finger-board,  a  note  could  be  instantaneously 

enough.    Probably  no  two  lyrists  tuned  alike.  sharpened  in  a  similar  manner  by  pressing  more 

My  experience  of  tuning  by  ear  is  quite  against  strongly. — Translator.l 

any  approach  to  the  accuracy  which  the  figures  %  [And  yet  a  quarter  of  a  Tone  is  between 

in  the  text  would  imply. — Translator.]  2  and  3  commas,  and  all  the  difficulties  of  tuning 

f  [Probably  the  effect  was  like  that  which  in  just  and  tempered  intonation  arise  from  in- 

I  heard  produced  by  Baja  R&m  P41  Singh  on  tervals  of  a  single  comma  or  less. — Translator,'] 
his  Sit4r.    Here  the  tone  of  the  note,  played  §  Even    Bellerman    is    of    this    opinion 

by  pressing  the  string  against  a  fret,  was  shar-  (Tonleiter  der  Qriechen,  p.   27).     Westphal, 

pened  a  quarter  of  a  Tone  by  sliding  the  finger  in  his  Fragmenten  der  Griechischen  Rhyth- 

along    the    fret   (thus  deflecting    the    string  miker^  p.  209,  has  collected  passages    from 

and  increasing  the  tension),  and  then  it  was  Greek  writers  proving  the  real  practical  use  of 

allowed  to  glide  on  to  the  proper  note  by  these  intervals.    According  to  Plutarch  (De 

straightening  the  string  without   repluoking  Mtisica^  pp.  38  and  39),  the  later  Greeks  had 

it.     I  determined  the  amount  of  sharpening  even  a  preference  for  these  surviving  archaic 

by  observing  the  distance  of  deflection,  and  intervals, 
then,  at  leisure,  measuring  by  my  forks  the 


Digitized  by 


Google 


266  VAEIOUS  DIATONIC  SCALES.  part  iu- 

natural  intonation  with  Pythagorean,  and  to  play  the  diatonic  mode  at  one  time 
after  the  method  of  Didymus  and  at  another  after  that  of  Ptolemy,  and  also  to 
make  other  deviations.  It  is  not  at  all  difficult  to  distingoish  the  difference  of  a 
comma  |^  in  the  intonation  of  the  different  degrees  of  the  scale,  when  well-laiown 
melodies  are  performed  in  different '  colourings,'  and  every  musician  with  whom  I 
have  made  the  experiment  has  immediately  heard  the  difference.  Melodic  passages 
with  Pythagorean  Thirds  have  a  strained  and  restless  effect,  while  the  natural 
Thirds  make  them  quiet  and  soft,  although  our  ears  are  hahituated  to  the  Thirds 
of  the  equal  temperament,  which  are  nearer  to  the  Pythagorean  than  to  the  natural 
intervals.  Of  course  where  delicacy  in  any  artistic  ohservations  made  with  the 
senses,  comes  into  consideration,  modems  must  look  upon  the  Greeks  in  general 
as  unsurpassed  masters.  And  in  this  particular  case  they  had  very  good  reason 
and  abundance  of  opportunity  for  cultivating  their  ear  better  than  ours.  From 
^  youth  upwards  we  are  accustomed  to  accommodate  our  ears  to  the  inaccuracies  of 
equal  temperament,  and  the  whole  of  the  former  variety  of  tonal  modes,  with  their 
different  expression,  has  reduced  itself  to  such  an  easily  apprehended  difference  aa 
that  between  major  and  minor.  But  the  varied  gradations  of  expressions  which 
modems  attain  by  harmony  and  modulation,  had  to  be  effected  by  the  Greeks  and 
other  nations  that  use  homophonic  music,  by  a  more  deUcate  and  varied  gradation  of 
the  tonal  modes.  Can  we  be  surprised,  then,  if  their  ear  became  much  more  finely 
cultivated  for  differences  of  this  kind  than  it  is  possible  for  ours  to  be  ? 

The  Greek  scale  was  soon  extended  to  an  octave.  Pythagoras  is  said  to  have 
been  the  first  to  estabUsh  the  eight  complete  degrees  of  the  diatonic  scale.  At  first 
two  tetrachords  were  connected  in  such  a  way  as  to  have  a  common  tone,  the  fjJtnj : 


.f^g  —  a^  l\}  —  c  —  d 


^  which  produced  a  scale  of  seven  degrees.    Then  this  scale  was  changed  into  the 
following  form : 

e  — / — g  —  a —  b  ^^  —  d  —  e 

and  thus  made  to  consist  of  a  tetrachord  and  a  trichord,  of  which  mention  has 
already  been  made  (p.  2576?).  Finally  Lichaon  of  Samos,  (according  to  Boethius,) 
or  Pythagoras,  (according  to  Nicomachus,)  completed  the  trichord  into  a  tetrachord, 
and  thus  established  a  scale  consisting  of  two  disjunct  tetrachords. 

The  diatonic  scale  thus  obtained  could  be  continued  either  way  at  pleasure  by 
adding  higher  and  lower  octaves,  and  it  then  produced  a  regularly  alternating 
series  of  Tones  and  Semitones.  But  for  each  piece  of  music  a  portion  only  of  this 
unlimited  diatonic  scale  was  employed,  and  the  tonal  systems  were  distinguished 
by  the  character  of  the  portions  selected. 

These  sectional  scales  might  be  produced  in  very  different  ways.    The  first 

%  practical  object  which  necessarily  forces  itself  on  attention,  as  soon  as  an  instm- 

ment  with  a  limited  number  of  strings,  like  the  Greek  lyre,  is  used  for  executing  a 

piece  of  music,  is,  of  course,  that  there  should  be  a  string  for  every  musical  tone 

required.     This  prescribes  a  certain  series  of  tones  which  must  be  provided  and 

tmied  on  the  instrument.    Now  as  a  rule  when  a  certain  series  of  tones  is  thus 

prescribed  as  a  scale  for  the  tuning  of  a  lyre,  no  question  is  raised  as  to  whether  a 

tonic  is  to  be  distinguished  or  not,  or  if  so  which  it  should  be.    A  tolerable  number 

of  melodies  may  be  found  in  which  the  lowest  tone  is  the  tonic :  others  in  which 

an  interval  below  the  tonic  is  touched  ;  and  others,  again,  in  which  the  Fifth  or 

Fourth  above  the  Octave  below  the  tonic  is  used.     This  is  the  kind  of  difference 

between  the  authentic  and  plagal  scales  of  the  middle  ages.    In  the  authentic 

scales  the  deepest  tone  of  the  scale,  in  the  plagal  its  Fifth  below  or  Fourth  above, 

was  the  tonic ;  thus  :  * — 

*  [See  Mr.  Rockstro's  article, '  Modes  Eocle-  on  *  Gregorian  Modes,'  vol.  i.  p.  625,  in  Grove's 
siastioal,'  vol.  ii.  p.  340,  and  Bev.  T.  Helmore's      Dictionary  of  Music,    What  Prof.  Helmholts 

Digitized  by  V^OOQIC 


CHAP.  XIV.  VARIOUS  DIATONIC  SCALES.  267 

First  Authentic  Eccleslastical  SoAiiE,  tonic  d. 
d  —  e — / — g  —  a  —  b  —  c  —  d 

V. ..p. / 

FouBTH  Plagal  Scale,  tonic  g. 


d  —  e  — /  —  g  —  a  —  b  —  c  —  d 

y , ^ 

The  scales  were  looked  upon  as  composed  of  a  Fifth  and  a  Fourth,  as  the 
braces  shew.  In  the  authentic  tone  the  Fifth  lay  below ;  in  plagal,  above. 
Now  if  we  have  nothing  else  before  us  but  a  scale  of  this  kind,  which  marks  out 
the  accidental  compass  of  a  series  of  melodies,  we  can  collect  but  Httle  respect- 
ing the  key.  Such  scales  themselves  may  be  fittingly  termed  accidental.  They 
comprise,  among  others,  the  medieval  plagal  scales.  On  the  other  hand,  those  f 
scales  which,  like  the  modem,  are  bounded  at  each  extremity  by  the  tonic,  may  be 
termed  essential.  Now  practical  needs  clearly  lead  in  the  first  place  to  accidental 
scales  alone.  When  a  lyre  had  to  be  tuned  to  accompany  the  human  voice  in 
unison,  it  was  indispensably  necessary  that  all  the  tones  required  should  be  present. 
There  was  no  immediate  practical  need  for  marking  the  tonic  of  a  song  sung  in 
miison,  or  even  to  become  fully  aware  that  it  had  a  tonic  at  all.  In  modem 
music,  where  the  structure  of  the  harmony  essentially  depends  on  the  tonic,  the 
case  is  entirely  different.  Theoretical  considerations  on  the  structure  of  melody 
could  alone  lead  to  distinguishing  one  tone  as  tonic.  It  has  been  already  mentioned 
in  the  preceding  chapter,  that  Aristotle,  as  a  writer  on  esthetics,  has  left  a  few 
notices  indicating  such  a  conception,  but  that  the  authors  who  have  specially 
written  on  music  say  nothing  about  it. 

In  the  best  times  of  Greece,  song  was  usually  accompanied  by  an  eight- stringed 
lyre,  tuned  so  as  to  embrace  an  Octave  of  tones  selected  from  the  diatonic  scale.  ^ 
These  were  the  following  : 

1.  Lydian c  —  d  —  e — / — g  —  a — b  —  c 

2,  Phrygian d  —  e — / — g  —  a — b  —  c  —  d 


3.  Doric e — / — g  —  a  —  b  —  c  —  d  —  e 

4.  Hypolydian        ....    / — g  —  a — b  —  c  —  d  —  e — / 


5.  Hypophrygian  (Ionic)  .        .    g  —  a  —  6  — c  —  d  —  e — / — g 


6.  Hypodoric  (Eolic  or  Locrian)      .    a — b  —  c  —  d  —  e — / — g  —  a 


7.  Mixolydian         ....     b  —  c  —  d  —  e — / — g  —  a  —  b  —  (c) 

Hence  any  one  of  the  tones  in  the  diatonic  scale  could  be  used  as  the  initial 
or  final  extremity  of  such  a  tonal  mode.  The  Lydian  and  Hypolydian  scales 
contain  Lydian,  the  Phrygian  and  Hypophrygian  contain  Phrygian,  and  the  Doric 
and  Hypodoric  contain  Doric  tetrachords.  In  the  Mixolydian  two  Lydian  tetra- 
chords  seem  to  have  been  assumed,  one  of  which  was  divided,  as  shewn  by  the  braces 
in  the  above  examples.* 

calls  the  ionic  was  termed  the  final.    What  on  the  piano  and  organ  in  equally  tempered 

was  the  exact  intonation  of  this  music  it  id  intonation,  as  their  ancestors  played  them 

perhaps  impossible  to  say.    Perhaps  we  may  in  meantone  intonation.     Bat  either  of  the 

assume  it  to  have  been  Pythagorean,  as  latter  admit  of  being  harmonised ;  not  so  the 

d2O4e90f2O4a2Oda2OAbQ0C2OAd.  former,  so  that  there  is  an  essential  difference. 

■^      "^  ^    ^        "»     ^  ^Translator.] 

Of  course,  modern  musicians  play  them  *  [By  a  reference  to  p.  263<2,  note,  it  will  be 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQ IC 


268 


VARIOUS  DIATONIC  SCALES. 


PART    III. 


The  scales  or  tropes  of  the  best  Greek  period  have  hitherto  been  considered  aB 
essential,  that  is,  the  lowest  tone  or  hypate  has  been  considered  as  the  tonic.  Bat 
I  cannot  find  any  definite  ground  for  this  assumption.  What  Aristotle  says,  as  we 
have  seen,  makes  the  middle  tone  or  mese,  function  as  the  tonic,  but  yet  it  cannot 
be  denied  that  other  attributes  of  our  tonic  belong  to  the  hypate.*  Whatever  may 
have  been  the  real  state  of  the  case,  whether  the  mese  or  hypate  be  regarded  as  the 
tonic,  whether  the  scales  be  considered  as  all  authentic  or  all  plagal,  it  is  extremely 
probable  that  the  Greeks,  among  whom  we  first  find  the  complete  diatonic  scale, 
took  the  Hberty  of  using  every  tone  of  this  scale  as  a  tonic,  just  as  we  have  seen 
that  every  one  of  the  five  tones  forming  the  scales  of  the  Chinese  and  Gaels 
occasionally  functions  as  a  tonic.  The  same  scales  are  also  found,  probably 
handed  down  immediately  by  ancient  tradition,  in  the  ancient  Christian  ecclesi- 
astical music. 

Hence  if  we  disregard  the  chromatic  and  enharmonic  scales,  and  the  apparently 
arbitrary  scales  of  the  Asiatics,  none  of  which  have  shewn  themselves  capable  of 
«r  further  development,t  homophonic  vocal  music  developed  seven  diatonic  scales, 
which  differ  from  one  another  in  about  the  same  way  as  our  major  and  minor 
scales.  These  differences  wiU  be  better  appreciated  by  making  them  all  begin 
with  the  same  tonic  c.t 

seen  that  this  paragraph  materially  alters  the 
intonation  from  what  would  result  from  a 
mere  beginning  of  each  mode  with  a  different 
note  of  the  Pythagorean  or  diatonic  scale.  I 
therefore  repeat  the  scales  as  defined  by  this 
paragraph  in  the  notation  explained  on  pp.  276a 
to  277a  and  note  •,  and  write  between  each  pair 
of  notes  the  number  of  cents  in  the  interval 
between  each  pair  of  notes,  which  will  be  found 
useful  in  future  comparisons.  These  scales 
^  should  be  traced  out  on  the  Duodenarium, 
App.  XX.  sect.  £.  art.  18.  They  cannot  be 
played  on  the  Harmonical. 

1.  Lydian,  c  182  di  204  ei  1 12  /  204  g  182  Aj  204 

5,  112  c 

2.  Phrygian,  d   182  ej  134  /*  182  g  204  a  182 

6,  134  c*  182  d 

3.  Doric,  ego  f  204  g  204  a  204  &  90  c  204 

d  204  e 

4.  Hypolydian,  /  204  g  182  ai  204  &i  112  c  182 

^,  2046,  112/ 

5.  Hypophrygian  (Ionic),  g  204  a  182  61  112 

c  204  d  182  e,  134/'  182  g 

6.  Hypodoric  (Eolic  or  Locrian),  a  204  b  90 

c  204  d  204  6  90  /  204  g  204  a 

7.  Mixolydian,  5i  112  c  182  di  204  0,  112/204 

g  182  a^  204  6,. — Translator.] 

*  B.  Westphal,  in  his  Oeschichte  der  alien 
gr  UTid  miltelalterlichen  Musik^  Breslau,  1864, 
>'  which  is  unfortunately  still  incomplete,  uses  the 

previous  citations  from  Aristotle,  to  frame  an 

hypothesis  on  the  tonic  and  final  cadence  of  the 

above  scales.    But  he  applies  the  remarks  of 

Aristotle  only  to  the  Doric,  Phrygian,  Lydian, 

Mixolydian  and  Locrian  scales,  and  not  to  the 

Eolic  and  Ionic,  which  were  also  known  at 

that  time,  although  the  ground  for  their  ex- 
clusion is  not  apparent.    In  the  first  four  of 

these  he  takes  the  mese  as  tonic  and  the 

hypate  as  the  terminal  tone.    In  those  scales 

distinguished  by  the  prefix  Hypo-t  the  hypate 

was  both  tonic  and  terminal;  but  in  those 

having  the  prefix  Syntono-t  the  hypate  was 

both  the  terminal  and  the  Third  of  the  tonic, 

and  the  same  was  the  case  perhaps  for  the 

Boeotian  scale,  which  is  only  mentioned  once. 

Hence  it  follows  that  the  minor  scale  of  A 

occurs  as  Doric  with  the  terminal  e,  as  Hypo- 


doric with  the  teiTninal  a,  as  Boeotian  with 
the  terminal  c.  Moreover  the  Ifixolydian 
would  be  a  minor  scale  of  JE7,  with  a  minor 
Second,  and  a  terminal  in  5;  the  Locrian  a 
minor  scale  of  D  with  a  major  Sixth,  and  a 
terminal  in  a ;  the  Phrygian,  Hypophrygian  or 
lastic,  and  the  Syntonoiastic,  major  scales  of 
Of  with  a  minor  Seventh,  the  terminals  being 
dy  g,  and  b  respectively.  Finally  the  Lydian, 
Hypolydian  and  Syntonolydian  would  be 
major  scales  of  F,  with  superfluous  Fourth, 
and  with  the  terminals  c,  /,  and  a  respectively. 
But  according  to  Westphal  the  normal  major 
scale  was  entirely  absent.  If  the  Ionic  were 
interpreted  according  to  the  words  of  Aristotle, 
it  would  yield  a  correct  major  scale.  The 
tonic  F  with  B  (instead  of  JBb)  as  its  Fourth, 
has  a  totally  impossible  appearance  to  modem 
musical  feeling. 

f  [In  India  there  is  a  highly  developed 
system  with  a  vast  variety  of  scales. — Trans- 
lator.] 

X  [Continuing  to  use  the  notation  of  p.  2686, 
note,  these  transposed  scales  may  be  written 
as  follows.  As  the  order  is  different  from  that 
in  p.  267c,  the  numbers  there  used  are  added 
in  n.  The  number  of  cents  in  each  interval 
will  complete  the  identification.  I  give  only 
the  ancient  Greek  names,  and  the  names  pro- 
posed by  Prof.  Helmholtz. 

1.  Lydian— Mode  of    the    First  (Major)   (i), 

c  182  di  204  6|  112  /  204  g  182  ai  204 
61  112  c. 

2.  Ionic    or    Hypophrygian  —  Mode    of    the 

Fourth  (5),  c  204  d  182  e^  112  /  204  g  182 
«!  134  &*b  182  c 

3.  Phrygian — Mode  of  the  minor  Seventh  (2), 

c  182  d,  134  e^b  182  /204  g  182  a.  134 
6'bi82c 

4.  Eolic— Mode  of  the  minor  Third  (Minor) 

(6),  c  204  d  90  eb  204  /  204  g  90  ab  204 
6b  204  c 

5.  Doric— Mode  of  the  minor  Sixth  (3),  c  90 

db  204  eb  204/ 204  9  90  ab  204  6b  204  e 

6.  Mixolydian — Mode  of  the  minor  Second  (7), 

c  ii2<2^b  I82  6b204/ii2  9*b204a*bi82 
6b  204  c 

7.  Syntonolydian— Mode  of  the  Fifth,  not  in 


Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


CHAP.  XIV. 


VARIOUS  DIATONIC   SCALES. 


269 


▲nolent  Qreek  Names 

Scales  beginning  with  <? 

Olarcan'B 

Ecclesiastical 

Names 

Proposed  new 
Name8« 

1.  Lydian  .... 

2.  Ionic  or  Hypophrygian  . 

3.  Phrygian 

4.  EoUc      .... 

5.  Doric      .... 

6.  r  Mixolydian   . 

7.  t  Syntonolydian 

c-d  -e  -/    -g  -a  -6  -</ 
c— <J  — e  -/    —g  —a  — 6b— c' 
c-d  -«b-/    -g  -a  — ftb-c/ 
c-d  -eb~/    -jT  -ab-6b-c' 

c— rfb— «b-/    — ^  — ab  — 6b— c' 
c— db— eb— /    — grb— ob~6b— c' 
c-d  — e  -ft -g  -a  -6   -c' 

Ionic 

Mixolydian 
Doric 
Eolic 

Phrygian 
Lydian       i 

Mode  of  the  :— 
First  {major) 
Fourth 

minor  Seventh 
minor  Third 

(minor) 
minor  Sixth 
minor  Second 
Fifth 

To  assist  the  reader  I  have  added  the  names  assigned  to  the  ecclesiastical 
modes  by  Olarean,  which  were  wrongly  distributed  among  the  scales  owing  to  his 
confdsing  the  older  tonal  modes  with  the  later  (transposed)  minor  Greek  scales,  ^ 
but  which  are  more  known  among  musicians  than  the  proper  Greek  names.  But 
I  shall  not  use  Glarean's  names  without  expressly  mentioning  that  they  refer  to  an 
ecclesiastical  mode.  It  would  be  really  better  to  forget  them  altogether.  The  old 
numerical  notation  of  Ambrose  was  much  more  suitable,  but  as  his  figures  have 
been  altered  again  and  do  not  suffice  for  all  modes,  I  have  ventured  to  propose  a 
new  nomenclature  in  the  above  table,  which  will  save  the  reader  the  trouble  of 
memorising  the  systems  of  Greek  names,  of  which  Glarean's  are  certainly  wrong, 
and  the  others  are  also  perhaps  not  quite  correctly  applied.  The  principle  of  the 
new  nomenclature  is  this.  By  '  the  mode  of  Fourth  of  C,'  is  meant  a  mode  of 
which  C  is  the  tonic,  but  which  has  the  same  signature  (or  additional  $  and  \} 
signs)  as  the  major  scale  formed  on  the  Fourth  of  the  diatonic  scale  beginning 
with  C  ;  that  is  on  F.  The  minor  Seventh,  minor  Third,  minor  Sixth,  and  minor 
Second  must  always  be  understood  as  the  intervals  intended  in  this  case.f  If  the 
major  intervals  were  selected  the  tonic  would  not  occur  in  their  scales.  Thus^ 
'  the  mode  of  the  minor  Third  of  C '  is  the  scale  with  the  tonic  C,  having  the 
signature  of  JSf)  major  (that  is  B{},  E\},  A\}),  because  E[}  is  the  minor  Third  of  C ; 
this  is  therefore  C  minor,  at  least  as  it  is  played  in  the  descending  scale.  I  hope 
the  reader  will  have  no  difficulty  in  understanding  what  is  meant  by  this  notation.^ 

This  was  tlie  tonal  system  in  the  best  times  of  Greek  art,  up  to  the  Macedonian 
empire.  Airs  were  at  first  Hmited  to  a  tetrachord,  as  is  still  often  the  case  in  the 
Boman  Catholic  Uturgy.  They  were  afterwards  extended  to  an  Octave.  Longer 
scales  were  not  necessary  for  singing,  as  the  Greeks  refused  to  employ  the  straining 
upper  notes,  and  unmetallic  deep  notes  of  the  human  voice.  Modem  Greek  songs, 
of  which  Weitzmann  has  made  a  collection,§  have  also  a  surprisingly  small  com- 
pass. If  Phrynis  (victor  in  the  Panathenaic  competitions,  B.C.  457)  added  a  ninth 
string  to  his  cithara,  the  chief  advantage  of  the  arrangement  was  to  allow  of 
passing  from  one  kind  of  scale  to  another. 

The  later  Greek  scale,  which  first  occurs  in  Euclid's  works  of  the  third  century  % 
B.C.,  embraces  two  Octaves,  thus  arranged : 


the  former  table  under  this  name,  but 
really  the  Hypolydian  (4),  c  204  d  182  6, 204 
/iS  112  g  182  a|  204  6,  112  c 
Befer    to    the    Duodenarium,    App.   XX. 
sect.  £.  art  iS.—Translator.] 

*  (If  we  subtract  each  of  the  nombers  in 
the  names  of  the  modes  here  proposed,  from 
9,  (reckoning  i  as  8  its  Octave,)  we  obtain  the 
nambers  on  p.  267c,  which  shew  the  number  of 
the  note  in  the  major  scale  determined  by  the 
signature,  on  which  the  special  scale  begins. 
Thus  as  9  less  7  is  2,  the  mode  of  the  minor 
Seventh  is  that  numbered  2  on  pp.  267c,  268c. 
If  we  call  the  major  scale,  when  reduced  to  a 
harmonisable  form,  i.  do,  2.  r«,  3.  tni,  4. /a, 
5.  so,  6. 2a,  7  tiy  then  these  transformed  modes 


may  be  called  with  the  Tonic  Sol-faists  the  do, 
re,  mi,  Ac,  modes  respectively. — Translator,] 

f  [The  qualification  minor  will  therefore 
be  always  used  in  this  translation,  and  has 
been  inserted  in  the  above  table. — Translator.] 

X  [In  App.  XX.  sect.  E.  No.  10,  I  have 
endeavoured  to  deduce  scales  for  harmonic  use, 
from  a  general  theory  of  harmony  which  de- 
termines the  precise  value  of  each  tone  as 
part  of  a  chord,  and  I  have  given  precise 
names  for  them,  there  exemplified.  This  har- 
monic deduction  of  scales  is  quite  independent 
of  the  historical  melodic  deduction  in  itie  text. 
— Translator.] 

§  Oeschichte  der  Oriechischen  Musik^ 
Berlin,  1855. 

Digitized  by  V^OOQIC 


270 


LATER  GREEK  SCALES. 


PART?   UU 


.  lowest  Tetrachord 


A     added  Tone 

B\ 

c 

d 

f      middle  Tetrachord 

9 
a 


Proslambanom'enos 
Tetra.  hyp'aton 

Tetra.  mes'on 


& 

d' 

fe' 

f 

9' 


disjunct  Tetra. 
T.  diezeug'mendn 

superfluous  Tetr. 
T,  hyperbolai'oTL 


a  \ 

& 

d' 


conjunct  Tetr. 
T,  synem'menon. 


This  scheme  gives  first  the  Hypodoric  [Eolic,  or  Locrian]  scale*  for  two 
Octaves,  and  then  an  added  tetrachord  which  introduces  a,Ij\}ia  addition  to  the  b, 
and  thus,  in  modem  language,  allows  of  modulation  from  the  principal  scale  into 
that  of  the  subdominant.t 

This  scale,  essentially  of  a  minor  character,  was  transposed,  and  thus  a  new 
series  of  scales  were  generated  that  correspond  with  the  (descending)  minor  scales 
of  modem  music.  To  these  were  applied  the  old  names  of  the  tonal  modes,  by 
giving  originally  to  each  minor  mode  the  name  belonging  to  that  tonal  mode 
which  was  formed  by  the  section  of  the  minor  scale  which  lay  between  the 
extreme  tones  of  the  Hypodoric  t  scale.  According  to  the  Greek  method  of  repre- 
senting the  notes,  these  extreme  tones  would  have  to  be  written  /../.  Their 
%  actual  pitch  was  probably  a  Third  lower.  Thus  the  minor  scale  of  D  was  called 
Lydian,  because  in  this  scale — 

d—e—  \f—g—a-'l^-'C'-d''e'-f\g—a-'l^^c—d 

the  section  of  the  scale  lying  between  the  extreme  tones  /and/  belonged  to  the 
Lydian  tonal  mode.  Li  this  way  the  old  names  of  the  tonal  modes  altered  their 
meaning  into  those  of  tonal  keys.  The  following  table  shews  the  correspondence 
of  these  names : — 


i)  Hypo-doric 

=  F    minor 

8)  Phrygian                      « 

9)  Eolio                             = 

C    minor 

2)  Hypo-ionio 

«  Ft  minor 

CI  minor 

(deeper  Hypo-phrygian) 

(deeper  Lydian) 

3)  Hypo-phrygian 

■a  Q    minor 

10)  Lydian                          « 

D    minor 

4)  Hypo-edic 

s  GS  minor 

II)  Hyper-dorio                  = 

Eb  minor 

(deeper  Hypo-lydian) 

(Mixo-lydian) 

5)  Hypo-lydian 

«  A    minor 

12)  Hyper-ionic                  « 
(higher  Mixo-lydian) 

E    minor 

6)  Doric 

=  B\>  minor 

13)  Hyper-phrygian 

/    minor] 

CB 

(Hyper-mizo-lydian) 

7)  lonio 

«  B    minor 

14)  Hyper-eolic                   « 

/8  minor 

3^ 

(deeper  Phrygian) 

15)  Hyper-lydian                = 

g     minor  ^ 

'S 

Within  each  of  these  scales  each  of  the  previously  mentioned  tonal  modes 
might  be  formed,  by  using  the  corresponding  part  of  the  scale.  Besides  this  it 
was  possible  to  pass  into  the  conjunct  tetrachord  and  thus  modulate  into  the  tonal 
key  of  the  subdominant. 

The  experiments  on  transposition  which  formed  the  basis  of  these  scales 


♦  [See  No.  6,  of  p.  267c,  text,  asBoming 
Pythagorean  intonation. — Translator.] 

t  Singularly  enough  this  species  of  musical 
scale  has  been  preserved  in  the  Zillerthalin 
Tyrol,  for  the  wood-harmonioon.  This  scale 
has  two  rows  of  bars.    One  forms  a  regular 


diatonic  scale  with  the  disjunct  tetrachord. 
The  other,  which  lies  deep,  has  the  conjunct 
tetrachord  in  its  upper  part. 

X  [This  seems  to  be  an  error  for  Hypo- 
lydian,  No.  4  of  p.  267c,  of  which  the  extreme 
tones  are  /  and  /. — Translator,] 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


CHAP.  XIV.  ECCLESIASTICAL  SCALES.  271 

shewed  that  the  Octave  might  be  considered  as  composed  approximativelj  of  twelve 
Semitones.  Even  Aristoxenus  knew  that  by  taking  a  series  of  twelve  Fifths  we 
reached  a  tone  that  was  at  least  very  near  'to  a  higher  Octave  of  the  initial  tone. 
Thus  in  the  series 

he  identified  e/jj^  with  /,  and  by  thus  closing  the  series  of  tones  he  obtained  a  cycle 
of  Fifths.  Mathematicians  denied  the  fact,  and  with  reason,  because  if  the  Fifths 
are  taken  perfectly  true,  ^  is  a  little  sharper  than  /.  For  practical  purposes, 
however,  the  error  was  quite  insensible,  and  might  be  justly  neglected  in  homo- 
phonic  music  in  particular.* 

This  closes  the  development  of  the  Greek  tonal  system.    Complete  as  is  our 
acquaintance  with  its  outward  form,  we  know  but  little  of  its  real  nature,  because 
the  examples  of  melodies  which  we  possess  are  not  only  few  in  number,  but  very  f 
doubtful  in  origin. 

Whatever  may  have  been  the  nature  of  tonahty  in  Greek  scales,  and  however 
numerous  may  be  the  questions  about  it  that  are  still  unresolved,  yet  so  far  as  the 
theory  of  the  general  historical  development  of  tonal  modes  is  concerned  we  leam 
all  we  want  from  the  laws  of  the  earhest  Christian  ecclesiastical  music,  wbich  at 
its  commencement  touched  upon  the  ancient  construction  as  it  died  out.  In  the 
fourth  century  of  our  era.  Bishop  Ambrose,  of  Milan,  established  four  scales  for 
ecclesiastical  song,  which  in  the  untransposed  diatonic  scale  were : 

First  mode  :      d  —  e  — / —  g  —  a  —  b  —  c  —  d  mode  of  the  minor  Seventh. 
Second  mode :  e  — / —  g  —  a  —  b  —  c  —  d  —  e  mode  of  the  minor  Sixth. 
Third  mode :    /  —  g  —  a  —  b  —  c  —  d  —  e  — /  mode  of  the  Fifth  (unmelodic). 
Fourth  mode  :  g  —  a  —  b  —  c  —  d  —  e  — / —  g  mode  of  the  Fourtb. 

The  variable  character  of  the  tone  b,  which  was  transmutable  into  l^  in  the  ^ 
later  Greek  scales,  remained,  and  produced  the  following  scales  : — 

First:  d  —  e — f—g  —  a  — 1\} — c — d  mode  of  the  minor  Third. 

a^^^A.  >.^««Au^^^    ( mode  of  the  minor  Second 

Second:  e — / — g  —  a  —  en  —  c  —  a — e   \.  i  j-  x 

•^      ^  ^     .  I  (unmelodic). 

Third :  / — g — a—  6^  —  c  — d  —  e  — /  mode  of  the  First  (major). 

Fourth:         g — a — ^  —  c  —  d — e — / — g   mode  of  the  minor  Seventh. 

There  can  be  no  doubt  that  these  Ambrosian  scales  are  to  be  regarded  as 
essential  (see  p.  2676),  for  the  old  rule  is  that  melodies  in  the  first  are  to  end  in  d, 
those  in  the  second  in  e,  those  in  the  third  in  /,  and  those  in  the  fourth  in  gr,  and 
this  marks  the  initial  tones  of  the  scale  as  tonics.  We  may  certainly  assume  that 
this  arrangement  was  made  by  Ambrose  for  his  choristers  as  a  practical  simplifica- 
tion of  the  old  musical  theory,  which  was  overloaded  with  an  inconsistent  nomen- 
clature. And  this  leads  us  to  conclude  that  we  were  right  in  conjecturing  that  the  ^ 
similar  older  Greek  scales  could  have  been  really  used  as  different  essential  scales. 

Pope  Gregory  the  Great  inserted  between  the  Ambrosian  essential  scales  the 
same  number  of  accidental  scales  (p.  267a),  called  plagal,  proceeding  from  the 
Fifth  to  the  Fifth  of  the  tonic.    The  Ambrosian  scales  were,  then,  called  authentic 

*  It  is  by  no  means  an  nnimportant  fact,  Bepresentations  of  sach  Antes  are  found  in 

for  oar  appreciation  of  the  Greek  scale,  that  a  the  very  oldest  Egyptian  monuments.    They 

flute  was  foand  in  the  royal  tombs  at  Thebes  are  very  long,  the  holes  are  all  near  the  end, 

in   Egypt   (now  in  the  Florentine    Museum,  and  hence  the  arms  must  have  been  greatly 

No.  ^88),  which,  according  to  M.  F^tis,  who  stretched,  giving  the  player  a  characteristic 

examined  it,  gave  an  almost  perfect  scale  of  position.    The  Greeks  can  scarcely  have  been 

Semitones  for  about  an  Octave  and  a  half ;  ignorant  of  this  scale  of  Semitones.    That  it 

namely,  was  not  introduced  into  their  theory  till  after 

Series  of  primes,  abb  b  &  c'id'  the  time  of  Alexander,  clearly  shows  the  pre- 

First  upper  partial  tones,  a'  b'b  b'  c"  c"U  d"  ferenoe  they  gave  to  the  diatonic  scale.    [M. 

Second  upper  partial  tones,  t!'  f  ft  ^'  g"t  a"  F6tis*s  deductions  must  be  treated  with  much 

Third  upper  partial  tones,  a"  6"b  b"  d"  c'"5  d"  caution.— Tmiwtotor.] 

Digitized  by  V^OOQIC 


272        KATIONAL  CONSTEUCTION  OF  DIATONIC  SCALES,     part     i. 


for  distinction.  The  existence  of  these  plagal  ecclesiastical  scales  helped  to  increase 
the  confusion  which  broke  over  the  ecclesiastical  scales  towards  the  end  of  the 
middle  ages,  as  composers  began  to  neglect  the  rules  which  fixed  the  terminal 
tones,  and  this  confusion  assisted  in  favouring  a  freer>  development  of  the  tonal 
system.  This  confusion  also  shewed,  as  we  remarked  in  the  last  chapter  (p.  2436), 
that  no  feeling  for  the  thorough  predominance  of  the  tonic  was  much  developed 
in  the  middle  ages.  But  a  step,  at  least,  was  made  in  advance  of  the  Greeks,  by 
recognising  as  a  rule  that  the  piece  should  close  on  the  tonic,  although  this  rule 
was  not  always  observed. 

Glarean  endeavoured  in  1547  to  reduce  the  theory  of  the  scales  to  order  again, 
in  his  Dodecachordon.  He  shewed  by  an  examination  of  the  musical  compositions 
of  his  contemporaries,  that  six,  and  not  four,  authentic  scales  should  be  distinguished, 
and  adorned  them  with  the  Greek  names  in  the  table  on  p.  269a.  Then  he  assumed 
^  six  plagal  scales,  and  hence  on  the  whole  distinguished  twelve  modes,  whence  the 
name  of  his  book.  Hence  down  to  the  sixteenth  century  essential  and  accidental 
scales  were  reckoned  as  parts  of  one  series.  Among  Glarean*s  scales  one  is 
unmelodic,  namely  the  mode  of  the  Fifth,  which  he  calls  the  Lydian.  There  are 
no  examples  of  these  to  be  found,  as  we  know  from  a  careful  examination  of 
medieval  compositions  made  by  Winterfeld,*  and  this  confirms  Plato's  opinion  of 
the  MixolydiEkn  and  Hypolydian  modes. 

Hence  there  remain  the  following  five  melodic  tonal  modes  applicable  strictly 
for  homophonic  and  polyphonic  vocal  music,  namely  : 


In  oar  Nomenclature 

Ajiclent  Greek 

Olarean's  Names 

Scale 

I 

2 

3 
4 
5 

Major  Mode     .... 
Mode  of  the  Fourth 
Mode  of  the  minor  Seventh     . 
Mode  of  the  minor  Third 
Mode  of  the  minor  Sixth 

Lydian 

Ionic 

Phrygian 

EoUc 

Doric 

Ionic 

Mixolydian 

Doric 

Eolic 

Phrygian 

H 


The  rational  construction  of  these  scales  when  extended  to  the  Octave  or  beyond 
the  Octave  results  from  the  principle  of  tonal  relationship  already  explained.f  The 
limits  of  the  extent  to  which  tones  related  in  the  first  degree  should  be  used,  are 
determined  by  the  necessity  of  avoiding  intervals  too  close  to  be  distinguished  with 
certainty.  The  larger  gaps  thus  left  have  to  be  filled  with  the  tones  most  nearly 
related  in  the  second  degree. 

The  Chinese  and  Gaels  made  the  whole  Tone  V  [=182  cents]  the  smallest 
interval.^:  The  Orientals,  as  we  have  seen,  still  retain  Quartertones.  The  Greeks 
experimented  with  them,  but  soon  gave  them  up  and  kept  to  the  Semitone  {f 
[=112  cents]  as  the  smallest. 

European  nations  have  followed  Greek  habits,  and  retained  the  Semitone  ff  as 
^  the  limit.  The  interval  between  ^  (f )  [=316  cents]  and  E  (|)  [=386  cents],  and 
between  A\}  (f)  [=814  cents]  and  A  (f)  [=884  cents],  in  the  natural  scale  is 
smaller,  being  ||  [=70  cents],  and  we  consequently  avoid  using  both  ^  and  E, 
or  both  A\}  and  A  in  tlie  same  scale.  We  thus  obtain  the  following  two  series  of 
intervals  between  the  most  nearly  related  tones  for  ascending  and  descending 
scales : 

Ascending :    c e    —  /  —  g  —  a c' 

*        if      I     V         * 

Descending :  c A\}  —  O  —  F  —  ^ C 


n     I 


10 


*  von  Winterfeld'B  Johannes  Gabrieli  und 
sein  Zeitalter,  Berlin,  1834,  vol.  i.  pp.  73  to 
iu8. 

t  [The  following  is  not  an  attempt  to 
restore  the  Greek  originals,  which  have  already 
been  treated,  but  to  form  harmonic  scales  on  the 


same,  and  these  are  obtained  by  another  pro- 
cess in  App.  XX.  sect.  E.  art.  ^.—TranslaUn'.] 
X  [I  have  found  much  smaller  intervals  in 
Chinese  instruments.    See  App.  XX.  sect.  K. 
— Translator,] 


Digitized  by 


Google 


CHAP.  XIV.  RATIONAL  CONSTRUCTION  OF  DIATONIC  SCALES.        273 

The  numbers  below  the  series  shew  the  intervals  between  the  two  tones  between 
which  they  are  placed.* 

It  is  at  once  seen  that  the  intervals  from  and  to  the  tonic  are  too  large,  and 
might  be  further  divided.  But  as  we  have  come  to  the  limits  of  relationship  in 
the  first  degree,  we  have  to  fill  these  gaps  by  tones  related  in  the  second  degree. 

The  closest  relationship  in  the  second  degree  is  necessarily  famished  by  the 
tones  most  nearly  related  to  the  tonic.  Among  these  the  Octave  stands  first.  The 
tones  related  to  the  Octave  of  the  tonic  are  of  course  the  same  as  those  related  to 
the  tonic  itself;  but  by  passing  to  the  Octave  of  the  tonic  we  obtain  the  descending 
in  place  of  the  ascending  scale,  and  conversely. 

Thus,  ascending  from  c  we  found  the  following  degrees  of  our  major  scale — 

c e— / —  g  —  a & 

But  taking  the  tones  related  to  c',  we  obtain — 

0 eb  — /—  9  —  c^ c' 

Hence  the  second  degree  of  relationship  to  the  tonic  gives  an  ascending  minor 
scale.  In  this  scale  e[>  is  given  as  the  major  Sixth  below  c'.  But  it  has  also  the 
weak  relationship  to  c  marked  by  5  :  6.  Now  we  found  that  the  sixth  partial  of  a 
compound  tone  was  clearly  audible  in  many  qualities  of  tone  for  which  the  seventh 
or  eighth  could  not  be  heard ;  for  example,  on  the  pianpforte,  the  narrower  organ 
pipes,  and  the  mixture  stops  of  the  organ.  Hence  the  relationship  expressed  by 
5  :  6  may  often  become  evident  as  a  natural  relationship  in  the  first  degree.  This, 
however,  could  scarcely  be  the  case  for  the  relationship  e  —  of)  or  5  :  8.  Hence  it 
is  more  natural  to  change  e  into  ^  than  a  into  at>  in  the  ascending  scale.  The 
latter,  0(7,  can  only  be  related  to  the  tonic  la  the  second  degree.  The  three 
ascending  scales  la  order  of  intelligibility  are,  therefore — f  ^ 

e e   — / — g  —  a c' 

c ^—f—g  —  a 0' 

c eb  — /—  g  —  (^ c' 

These  distinctions  based  on  a  relationship  in  the  second  degree,  through  the 
medium  of  the  Octave,  are  certainly  very  slight,  but  they  make  themselves  felt 
in  the  well-known  transformation  of  the  ascending  minor  scale,  to  which  these 
distinctions  clearly  refer. 

Descending  from  0,  instead  of  the  relations  in  the  first  degree,  given  in 

c A\^—G  —  F  —  E\} C 

we  may  assume  relations  in  the  second  degree,  that  is  of  the  deeper  0,  and 
obtain  „ 

c A  —  G  —  F  —  E G  ^ 

Tn  the  latter,  A  is  connected  with  the  initial  tone  by  the  distant  relationship  in  the 
first  degree,  5  :  6,  and  E  only  by  a  relationship  in  the  second  degree.  Hence  the 
third  descencUng  scale 

c A  —  G  —  F  —  E\} C 

which  we  also  found  as  an  ascending  scale.  For  descending  scales  we  have  there- 
fore the  following  series.^ 

*  [With  the  subsequent  notation  and  inter-  c  386  e^  112/  204  g  182  a,  316  c' 

yals  expressed  in  cents :  c  316  e* bi82 /  204  9  182  A)  316  (/ 

C386.,  112/204^  182a,  316C  c3i6e'bi82/204(7ii2a'b386C 
c  386  i'b  112  G  2b4  F  182  £»b3i6  C  Translator.] 

Translator,']  +  [These  are  the  same  three  scales  as  in 
f  [With  the  subsequent  notation  and  inter-      the  last  note,  read  backwards.— 2Vans^^.J 
vals  in  cents ; 


Digitized  by 


^.jo^gle 


274         EATIONAL  CONSTEUCTION  OF  DIATONIC  SCALES,    part  in. 

c ^.A\}'-G  —  F  —  E^ G 

c A   —G^F  —  E\) G 

c A   —G  —  F  —  E G 

Generally,  since  all  Octaves  of  the  tonic,  distant  or  near,  higher  or  lower,  are 
80  closely  related  that  they  can  be  almost  identified  with  it,  all  higher  and  lower 
Octaves  of  the  individual  degrees  of  the  scale  are  almost  as  closely  related  to  the 
tonic,  as  those  of  the  next  adjacent  tonic  of  the  same  name. 

Next  to  the  relations  of  the  Octave  &  of  c,  follow  those  of  g^  the  Fifth  above, 
and  F  the  Fifth  below  e.  We  must  therefore  proceed  to  study  their  effect  in  the 
construction  of  the  scale.  Let  us  begin  with  the  relations  of  g,  the  Fifth  above 
the  tonic* 

ABCENDINa  SCALES. 

Belated  to  c :    c e  — / —  g  —  a c' 

H  Belated  to  g  :    c  d  fi^  —    —  g b  —  c' 

Uniting  the  two,  we  have — 

i)  The  Major  Scale  (Lydian  mode  of  the  ancient  Greeks) : 
c  —  d  —  e — / — g  —  a  —  h  —  cf 

I     I     f     J     f      f     V     2 

The  change  of  e  into  e[>  is  here  facilitated  by  its  second  relationship  to  g.  This 
gives — 

2)  The  Ascending  Minor  Scale  : 

c  —  d  —  4>  — / — 9  —  ^  —  *  —  ^ 

I     I      *      *     *     *    V     ^ 

DESOENDING   SCALES. 

^  Belated  toe:    c il^— G^  —  i^  —  E^ G 

Belated  to  flf:    cJBb G E^  —  D—G 

giving:— 

if  The  Descending  Minor  Scale  (Hypodoric  or  Eolic  mode  of  the  ancient 
Greeks — our  mode  of  the  minor  Third) : 

c  —  S^  —  A)^  —  G  —  F^E(^  —  D  —  G 
2       *        f         I       $        t         I       I 
or  in  the  mixed  scale,  changing  A\^  into  A, 

*  [In  the   complete    notation,  and   mfh  This  is  different  from  the  Greek  Phrygian, 

intervals  in  eents,  these  scales  are :  p.  26Sd\  note  {,  No.  3,  in  the  two  last  intervals. 

Ascending  Scales.  It  is  i  C  ma.mi.mi.  {ibid,  VH.). 

Related  to  c :  c  386  c,  1 12  /  204  ^  182  a,  316  c'  Ascending  Scales. 

Belated  to  g:  c  20^diJ2e^b  386  (7386  &i  112  c'  Related  to  c :  c  386  e,  112  /  204  ^  182  a,  316  c' 

1)  Major  Scale:  Belated   to   F:   c  182   d,  316  /  386   a,  112 
^  c  204  d  182  0,  112/204  g  182  a,  204  6,  112  c  6b  204  c^ 

"  This  is  not  quite  the  Greek  Lydian,  see  p.  2684',  5)  Mode  of  the  Fourth : 

note  %,  No.  i.  It  is  i  C7  ma.TDa.nia.  of  App.  XX.  c  182  (2,  204  d,  1 12  /  204  ^  182  a,  1126b  204  t/ 

sect.  E.  art.  9, 1.  l^is  is  not  quite  Greek  Ionic  or  Hypophrygian, 

2)  The  Ascending  Minor  Scale :  p.  268^',  note  :{:,  No.  2.    It  is  5  ^  majnaona. 
c  204  (2  112  e'b  182  /  204  (7  182  a,  204  61  112  c'  {ibid  I.). 

This  is  I  C  majni.ma.  {ibid,  HI.),  6)  New  form  of  mode  of  the  minor  Seventh  : 

Descending  Scales.  c  182  d  i^  .ibi82/2CH^  182  a,  112  6b204c' 

A/&Bi.j9rii^xx«u  wvai^o.  rpjj^g  IS  5  ^  ma.majm.  {ibid,  V.). 

Belated  to  c:   c  386  -4>b  112  G  204  F 182  ^       ^            ^    ^       ' 

^'b  316  C  Descending  Scales. 

Belated  to  g:  c  182  B^b  316  O  386  E^b  Ii2  Belated   to   c:    C386  A^b  112  0  204  F  182 

D204G  E^bsiSC 

3)  The  Descending  Minor  Scale:  Belated  to  F:    0204  Bb  112  ^1386  F386 
c  182  B'b204  A^b  112  G204  F  182  Jfi?»b  112  D»b  112  C 

D  204  C  7)  Mode  of  the  minor  Sixth: 

This  is  not  quite  the  Greek  Eolic,  see  p.  26^',  c  204  Bb  182  A^b  112  G  204  JP  182  ^b  204 

note  t,  No.  4.  It  is  i  C  mi.mi.mi.  {ibid.  VIII.).  D'  b  1 12  C 

4)  Mode  of  the  minor  Seventh :  This  is  not  quite  the  Greek  Doric,  p.  26Sd\ 
c  182  B>b  134  A^  182  G  204  F  182  E^b  112  note  J,  No.  5.   It  is  5  F mi.ini jni. (i6i(i.  VIII-). 

D  204  C  — Translator.] 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


CHAP.  XIV.  RATIONAL  CONSTBUCTION  OF  DIATONIC  SCALES.        275 

4)  Mode  of  the  minor  Seventh  (ancient  Greek  Phrygian) : 

c  —  Bi}  —  A  —  G  —  F  —  Ei}  —  D—G 
2        »        f        I       *        *        I         I 
On  examining  the  relations  of  F,  the  Fifth  below  the  tonio  c,  the  following 
scales  result : 

ABCENDma  SCALES. 

Belated  to  c :    c e  — /  —  g  —  a c' 

Related  to -P:   c—d / a  — Jt>  — (/ 

This  gives — 

5)  The  mode  of  the  Fourth  (ancient  Greek  Hypophrygian  or  Ionic) : 

c  —  <Z  —  e  — / — g  —  a  —  l\}  —  & 
I      V      f      f      t       *       V       2 

By  changing  e  into  e\},  we  again  obtain —  ^ 

6)  TTie  mode  of  the  mdnor  Seventh,  but  with  a  different  determination  of  the 
intercalary  tones  d  and  b\},  from  those  in  No.  4  : 

c  —  d  —  e\} — / — g  —  a  —  b\}  —  (/ 
I      V     *        I      I       *       V       2 

DESCENDINO  SCALES. 

Belated  toe:    c A\}—a  —  F—Ej;} C 

Belated  ioF:  c  —  Bl;}  —  A F JD\}—C 

giving  :— 

7)  The  mode  of  the  minor  Sixth  (ancient  Greek  Doric) : 

c  —  Bi;}  —  J\}  —  G  —  F  —  E\;}  —  JD\}—C 

2    V      i     i    i     i     n     ^  H 

In  this  way  the  melodic  tonal  modes  of  the  ancient  Greeks  and  Christian 
Church  have  all  been  rediscovered  by  a  consistent  method  of  derivation.  As  long 
as  homophonic  vocal  music  is  alone  considered,  all  these  tonal  modes  are  equally 
justified  in  their  construction. 

The  scales  have  been  given  above  in  the  order  in  which  they  are  most  naturally 
deduced.    But,  as  we  have  seen,  each  of  the  three  scales 

b e  — /  —  g  —  a c' 

c i\}—  f—g  —  a & 

c ^—  f—g  —  c^ c' 

can  be  played  either  upwards  or  downwards,  although  the  first  is  best  suited  to 
ascending  and  the  last  to  descending  progression,  and  hence  the  gaps  of  any  one 
of  them  may  be  filled  up  with  either  the  relations  of  F  or  the  relations  of  g,  or 
even  one  gap  with  those  of  F  and  the  other  with  those  of  g.  IT 

The  pitch  numbers  of  the  tones  directly  related  to  the  tonic  are  of  course  fixed  * 
and  unchangeable,  because  they  are  given  by  the  condition  that  the  tones  should 
form  consonances  with  the  tonic,  and  are  thus  more  strictly  determined  than  by  any 
more  distant  connection.  On  the  other  hand,  the  intercidary  tones  related  in  the 
second  degree  are  by  no  means  so  precisely  fixed. 

Taking  c=i,  we  have  for  the  Second — 

i)  the  d  derived  from  g  =f ,  [=204  cents] 

2)  the  d  derived  from  /  =  V  =ff  x  |,  [==  1 82  cents] 

3)  the  e2t>  derived  from/=|J,  [=112  cents] 

*  Thus  I  eannoi  agree  with  Hanptmann,  damental  bass  d.    But  this  would  indicate  a 

in  allowing  a  Pythagorean  a,  the  Fifth  above  (2,  distinct  modulation  into  O  major,  which  is  not 

in  the  ascending  minor  scale  of  c.    d'Alembert  required  when  the  nataral  relations  of  the 

introdnoes  the  same  tone  even  in  the  major  tones  to  the  tonic  are  preserved.    See  Haupt- 

scale,  by  passing  from  ^  to  6  through  the  fun-  mann,  Hamumik  und  Metrik^  p.  60. 

Digitized  by  VjCJC3QIC 


276         RATIONAL  CONSTRUCTION  OP  DIATONIC  SCALES,    pabt  m. 

and  for  the  Seventh—^ 

i)  the  6  derived  from  gr  =  Y,  [=1088  cents] 

2)  the  6l>  derived  from  g  =§,  [=1018  cents] 

3)  the  ^b  derived  from  /  =  V  =|f  x  f ,  [=996  cents] 

Hence  while  b  and  d\}  are  given  with  certainty,  l^  and  d  are  uncertain.  Either  of 
them  may  he  distant  from  the  tonic  hy  the  major  Tone  f  [=204  cents]  or  the 
minor  Tone  y,  [=182  cents]. 

In  order  henceforth  to  mark  this  difference  of  intonation  with  certainty  and 
without  amhiguity,  we  will  introduce  a  method  of  distinguishing  the  tones  deter- 
mined by  a  progression  of  Fifths,  from  those  given  by  the  relationship  of  a  Third 
to  the  tonic.  We  have  already  seen  that  these  two  methods  of  determining  the 
tones  lead  to  somewhat  different  pitches,  and  hence  in  accurate  theoretical  re- 
f  searches  both  kinds  of  tones  must  be  kept  distinct,  although  in  modem  music  they 
are  practically  confused.  4 

The  idea  of  this  notation  belongs  to  Hauptmann,  but  as  the  capital  and  small 
letter  which  he  uses,  and  which  I  also,  in  consequence,  employed  in  the  first  edition 
of  this  book,  have  a  different  meaning  in  our  method  of  writing  tones,  I  now 
introduce  a  slight  modification  of  his  notation. 

Let  C  be  the  initial  tone,  and  write  *  its  Fifth  (?,  the  Fifth  of  this  Fifth  jD,  and 
so  on.  In  the  same  way  let  the  Fourth  of  G  be  F,  the  Fourth  of  this  Fourth  £\} 
and  so  on.  In  this  way  we  have  a  series  of  Tones,  here  written  with  simple 
capitals,  all  distant  from  each  other  by  a  perfect  Fifth  or  a  perfect  Fourth  :  f 

Bj;}±F±C±G±D±A±E,&o. 

The  pitch  of  every  tone  in  the  whole  series  is,  therefore,  known  when  that  of  any 
one  is  known. 
If       The  major  Third  of  (7,  on  the  other  hand,  will  be  expressed  by  Ei,  that  of  F 
by  ill,  ^^^^  ^  0^*    Hence  the  series  of  tones 

is  a  series  of  alternate  major  and  minor  Thirds.  It  is  therefore  clear  that  the 
Tones 

Di±Ai±Ei±By±  Fjf^,  &c., 

also  form  a  series  of  perfect  Fifths. 

We  have  already  found  that  the  tone  D, ,  that  is  the  minor  Third  below  or  major 
Sixth  above  jP,  is  lower  in  pitch  than  the  tone  D,  which  would  be  reached  by  a 
series  of  Fifths  from  F,  and  that  the  difference  of  pitch  is  that  known  as  a  comma, 
the  numerical  value  of  which  is  |^,  or  musically  about  the  tenth  part  of  a  whole 
Tone.t  Since,  then,  D-±A  and  I>i±ili  are  both  perfect  Fifths,  A  must  be  also  a 
%  comma  higher  than  A  | ,  and  so  also  every  letter  with  an  inferior  number,  as  i ,  2, 3,  &c., 
attached  to  it,  will  represent  a  tone  which  is  i,  2,  3,  &c.,  commas  lower  in  pitch  than 

♦  Die  Naiur  der  Harmonik  und  Meirik^  quently,  and  change  (— )  into  (  +  )  for  the 

Leipzig,  1853,  pp.  26  and  following.    I  cannot  major  Third  of  386  cents.    In  the  case  of 

but  join  wi&  C.  E.  Naamann  in  expressing  my  Fifths  which  consist  of  a  major  and  a  minor 

regret  that  so  many  delicate  musical  appercep-  Third    702  =  386  +  316    cents,  the    symbol  ia 

tions  as  this  work  contains,  should  have  been  properly  ±.  which  I  here  also  take  the  liberty 

needlessly   buried  under  the  abstruse  termi-  to  use.    For  other  intervals  I  shall  use  (...)  for 

nology  of  Hegelian  dialectics,  and  hence  have  (— ),  and  generally  give  the  precise  interval  in 

been  rendered  inaccessible  to  any  large  circle  cents  elsewhere.    I  trust  that  this  change  will 

of  readers.  be  found  suggestive  as  well  as  convenient,  and 

f  [Prof.  Helmholtz  uses  (—)  between  the  may  therefore  not  be  considered  presamptuons. 

letters  in  all  such  cases.    I  have  taken  the  — Translator,'] 

liberty  from  this  place  onwards,  whenever  a  %  [The  comma  81  :  80  is  just   over   2i4 

line  or  combination  of  Thirds  occurs  to  leave  cents,  for  which  I  use  22  cents,  see  App.  X]^ 

( — )  only  in  the  just  minor  Thirds  of  316  cents,  sect.  A.  art.  4,  and  sect.  D.  Hence  a  major  tone 

to  use  (  I  )  in  the  Pythagorean  minor  Thirds  of  204  cents  contains  about  9^^  commas. — 

of  294  cents,  as  Prof.  Helmholtz  does  subse-  Translator,'] 


Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


CHAP.  XIV.  INTEODUOTION  OF  MOKE   PEECISE   NOTATION. 


277 


that  represented  by  the  same  letter  with  no  inferior  number  attached,  as  is  easily 
seen  by  carrying  on  the  series. 

A  major  triad  will  therefore  be  written  thus : 

and  a  minor  triad 

Ai-C+Ei  or  Ci-^+<?i 

Now  if  we  lay  it  down  as  a  rule  that  as  every  inferior  figure,  i,  2,  3,  &o.,  depresses 
its  tone  by  the  i,  2,  3,  &c.,  conmia,  every  superior  figure,  i,  2,  3,  &c.,  shall  raise  its 
tone  by  the  same  i,  2,  3,  &c.,  commas,  we  may  write  the  major  triad  as 

c+ei-g  or  c^  +  e-g^ 


and  the  minor  triad  as 


or  even 


c-e^l^+g  or  Ci-t^-^gi, 
c^-e^-g^  or  Ca-eib-gfj.* 


The  three  series  of  Tones  directly  related  to  C  are  consequently  to  be  written 
thus  : 

C El  ^F-G-Ai c 

and  the  intercalary  tones  are — 

Between  the  tonic  and  Third,  jD,  Dj,  or  D*t>. 
Between  the  Sixth  and  Octave,  Bi  and  £\}  or  B^\}. 

Consequently  the  melodic  tonal  modes  of  the  ancient  Greeks  and  old  Christian 
Church  are,t 


*  In  the  ist  [German]  edition  of  this 
book,  as  in  Hanptmann's,  the  small  letters 
were  supposed  to  be  a  comma  lower  than  the 
capital  letters,  and  a  stroke  above  or  below  the 
letters  was  only  occasionally  used  for  raising 
or  depressing  the  pitch  by  two  commas.  Hence 
a  major  triad  was  written  C  —  e— Ootc  —  E 
—  J ;  a  minor  triad  a  —  C  —  c,  or  4  —  c  —  -B, 
&c.  The  notation  used  here  [in  fhe  3rd  and 
4th  German  and  the  ist  English  editions] 
and  also  in  the  French  translation  is  due  to 
Herr  A.  v.  Oettingen,  and  is  much  more  readily 
comprehended.  [Herr  v,  Oettingen's  notation 
of  lines  above  and  below,  which  was  at  Prof. 
Helmholtz's  request  retained  in  the  ist  Eng- 
lish edition  of  this  translation,  was  found 
extremely  inconvenient  for  the  printer,  and 
actually  delayed  the  work  three  months  in 
pa<»8ing  through  the  press.  I  have  now  for 
some  years  employed  the  very  easy  substitute 
here  introduced.  By  referring  to  the  table 
called  the  Duodenarium,  in  App.  XX.  sect.  E. 
art.  18,  where  this  new  notation  is  systemati- 
cally carried  out  for  117  notes,  the  whole  bear- 
ing of  it  will  be  better  appreciated.  Another 
notation  which  I  had  used  formerly,  and  into 
which  I  translated  Herr  v.  Oettingen 's  in  the 
footnotes  to  the  ist  edition  of  this  translation, 
and  employed  in  Table  IV.,  there  correspond- 
ing to  my  present  Duodenarium,  is  conse- 
quently abandoned,  and  is  now  only  mentioned 
to  account  for  the  difference  in  notation  be- 
tween the  two  editions  of  this  translation. 
The  spirit  of  Herr  v.  Oettingen's  notation  is 
therefore  retained,  while  its  use  has  been 
rendered  typographically  convenient. — Trans- 
lator.] 

t  [This  variation  of  the  intercalary  tones 
really  amounts  to  a  change  of  mode,  so  that 


the  names  used  in  the  text  become  ambiguous.  ^ 
This  difficulty  is  overcome  by  the  trichordal 
notation  proposed  in  App.  XX.  sect.  E.  art.  9. 
i)  The  major  mode  of  C  with  D,  has  the  3 
major  chords  2?^+ .4, -C,  C  tE.-O,  Q-k-B^- 
D  and  is  i  C  ma.ma.ma.  But  with  D^  in 
place  of  D,  it  has  the  3  minor  chords  D^—F-{- 
ii„  il, -  O  +  J5|,  £,  -  G  +  jB,  (of  which  the  two 
last  belong  also  to  the  first  form),  and  is  there- 
fore 3  A^  mi.mi.mi.  This  is  a  related,  but  very 
different,  mode. 

2)  The  mode  of  the  Fourth,  as  it  stands  in 
the  first  line,  is  not  trichordal,  but  by  using 
D  and  jB'b  it  has  the  3  chords  JP-»-4,-C, 
G-k-E^-Q,  0''B^\}+D,  and  is  hence  i  0 
ma.ma.mi.  If  we  take  I),  and  Bb  it  has  the 
3  chords  Bb  +  Di-F,  F+A^-C,  C  +  J5,- 
G,  and  is  hence  5  F  ma.ma.ma.  With  both 
D,  and  B'b  it  is  again  not  trichordal. 

3)  The  mode  of  the  minor  Seventh.    If  we  ^ 
take  the  upper  line  as  it  stands,  this  is  also  not 
trichordal.    But  if  we  use  D  and  B*  b,  it  has  the 

3  chords  F+A^-C,  C-E'b  +  Q,  G-B'b  + A 
and  is  hence  I  C  ma.mi.mi.  If  we  take  D, 
and  Bb ,  the  3  chords  are  Bb  +Di-F,  F+A^ 
-C,  C-^'b  +  G,  and  the  scale  is  5  JP 
ma.majni.  With  D^  and  £'  b  the  scale  is  again 
not  trichordal. 

4)  Mode  of  the  minor  Third.  The  first 
line  as  it  stands  is  not  tri^ordal.  Taking 
D  and  S'b  the  3  chords  are  F-A^b-i-  C,C- 
£'b  +  G,  G-B'b  +  D,  and  the  scale  is  i  C 
mi.mi.mi.  Taking  i>,  and  Bb  the  3  chords  are 
Bb-^Di^F.F-A^b  +  CrC-E^b  +G,andthe 
scale  is  5  JP  ma.mijni.  With  Dj  and  B'b 
again  the  scale  is  not  trichordal. 

5)  Mode  of  the  minor  Sixth.  The  first 
line  as  it  stands  gives  the  3  chordsBb— i)'b  + 
^,  ^-^'b+C,  C--E'b  +  G,  and  the  scale  is 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


278  INTBODUCTION  OF  MOBE  PRECISE  NOTATION,     pabt  m.      | 

i)  Majob  mode, 

C...D.,.Ei...F.,.G.,.Ai,..Bi,..c 
Di 

2)  Mode  op  the  Foubth, 

3)  Mode  of  the  minob  Seventh, 

4)  Mode  of  the  mikob  Thibd, 

^   5)  Mode  of  the  minob  Sixth, 

J3»b 

By  this  notation,  then,  the  intonation  is  always  exactly  expressed,  and  the  kind 
of  consonance  which  each  tone  makes  with  the  tonic,  or  the  tones  related  to  it  is 
clearly  shewn. 

In  the  ancient  Greek  Pythagorean  intonation  these  scales  would  have  to  be 
written: 

Major  mode--  C...D...E,.,F.,.a...A..,B...C. 

and  the  others  in  a  similar  manner,  all  with  letters  of  the  same  kind,  belonging 
to  the  same  series  of  Fifths.* 

In  the  formulae  here  given  for  the  diatonic  tonal  modes,  the  intonation  of  the 
Second  and  Seventh  is  partly  undetermined.    In  these  cases  I  have  given  D  the 

^  preference  over  D^,  and  ^  the  preference  over  B^\},  because  the  relationship  of 
the  Fifth  is  closer  than  that  of  the  Third ;  but  ^  and  D  stand  in  the  relation  of 
the  Fifth  respectively  to  F,  (?,  the  tones  nearest  related  to  the  tonic,  while  Dj  and 
B^\}  are  only  in  the  relation  of  the  minor  Third  to  F  and  G.  But  this  reason  is 
certainly  not  sufficient  entirely  to  exclude  the  two  last  tones  in  homophonic  vocal 
music.  For  if  in  a  melodic  phrase,  the  Second  of  the  tonic  came  into  close  con- 
nection with  tones  related  to  jP— for  instance,  if  it  fell  between  F  and  il|,  or 
followed  them — an  accurate  singer  would  certainly  find  it  more  natural  to  use  the 
Di,  which  is  directly  related  to  F  and  Ai,  than  |the  D  which  is  related  to  them 
only  in  the  third  degree.  The  slightly  closer  relationship  of  the  latter  to  the 
tonic  could  scarcely  give  the  decision  in  its  favour  in  such  a  case. 

This  ambiguity  in  the  intercalary  tones  cannot,  I  think,  be  considered  as  a 
fault  in  the  tonal  system,  since  in  our  modem  minor  mode,  the  Sixth  and  Seventh 
of  the  tonic  are  often  altered,  not  merely  by  a  comma,  but  by  a  whole  Semitone, 

f  according  to  the  direction  of  the  melodic  progression.  We  shall  find,  however, 
more  decisive  reasons  for  the  use  of  D  in  place  of  Di  in  the  next  chapter,  when  we 
pass  from  homophonic  music  to  the  influence  of  harmonic  music  on  the  scales. 

The  account  here  given  of  the  rational  construction  of  scales  and  the  corre- 
sponding intonation  of  intervals,  deviates  essentially  from  that  given  to  the  Greeks 
by  Pythagoras,  which  has  thence  descended  to  the  latest  musical  theories,  and 
even  now  serves  as  the  basis  of  our  system  of  musical  notation.  Pythagoras 
constructed  the  whole  diatonic  scale  from  the  following  series  of  Fifths  : — 

F±C±G±D±A±E±B, 

5  F  mijnijni.    If  we  use  B^b   in  place  of  distinct,  though   purposely  confused  in  the 

£b,  the  3  chords  are  i)'b  +  JP—>i*b,  il'b  +  C—  nomenclature  of  the  text,  apparently  as  aa      ^ 

E^bi  E^b  +0~B^b,  and  the  scale  is  3  A^b  accommodation  to  the  usual  tempered  nota- 

nujnijni.  Hon.— TrmislatorJ] 

The  modes  formed  by  taking  one  inter-  *  [In  this  case  the  intonation   becomes 

calary  tone  or  the  other  are  therefore  quite  altogether  different.  -  Translator.} 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


CHAP.  XIV.  INTRODUCTION  OP  MORE   PRECISE  NOTATION. 


279 


and  oalcnlated  the  intervals  from  it  as  they  have  been  given  above.  In  his  diatonic 
scale  there  are  but  two  kinds  of  small  intervals,  the  whole  Tone  f ,  [s  204  cents] 
and  the  Limma  f4§>  [=  9<>  cents].* 

In  this  series  if  C  be  taken  as  the  tonic,  A  would  be  related  to  the  tonic  in  the 
Third  degree,  E  in  the  Fourth,  and  B  in  the  Fifth.  Such  a  relationship  would  be 
absolutely  insensible  to  any  ear  that  has  no  guide  but  direct  sensation. 

A  series  of  Fifths  may  certainly  be  tuned  on  any  instrument,  and  continued  as 
far  as  we  please ;  but  neither  singer  nor  hearer  could  possibly  discover  in  passing 
from  c  to  e  that  the  latter  is  the  fourth  from  the  former  in  the  series  of  Fifths.  Even 
in  a  relation  of  the  second  degree  through  Fifths,  as  of  c  to  d,  it  is  doubtful  whether 
a  hearer  can  discover  the  relation  of  the  tones.  But  in  this  case  when  we  pass 
from  one  tone  to  the  other  we  can  imagine  the  insertion  of  a  silent  g,*  so  to  speak, 
forming  the  Fourth  below  c,  and  the  Fifth  below  d,  and  thus  establish  a  comiection, 
for  the  mind's  ear  at  least,  if  not  for  the  body's.  This  is  probably  the  meaning  to  ^ 
be  attached  to  Rameau*s  and  d'Alembert's  explanation  that  a  singer  effects  the 
passage  from  ciodhy  means  of  the  fundamental  bass  G.  If  the  singer  does  not 
hear  the  bass  note  G  at  the  same  time  as  d,  he  cannot  possibly  bring  his  d  into 
consonance  with  that  G ;  but  the  melodic  progression  may  certainly  be  facilitated 
by  conceiving  the  existence  of  such  a  tone.  This  is  a  well-known  means  for 
striking  the  more  difficult  intervals,  and  is  often  applied  with  advantage.  But  of 
course  it  completely  fieuls  when  the  transition  has  to  be  made  between  tones  widely 
separated  in  the  series  of  Fifths.f 


*  [The  fact  that  the  Greek  scale  was 
derived  from  the  tetrachord,  or  divisions  of 
the  Fourth,  and  not  the  Fifth,  leads  me  to 
suppose  that  the  tuning  was  founded  on  the 
Fourth  and  not  the  Fifth.  On  proceeding  up- 
wards from  C  by  Fourths,  we  get  C  F  Bb  Eb 
Ab Db  Ob  Cb  Fb  Bbb  Ebb  Abb  Dbb,  and 
on  proceeding  dou^wards  we  get  C  G  D  A  E. 
Now  these  notes  after  Gf  b  in  the  first  series,  are 
precisely  those  of  Abdulqadir,  written  as  ^j5,  ^E^ 
M,  *Di  G,  *C,  on  p.  2826,  according  to  the  no- 
tation explained  on  p.  2816,  note  *.  Of  course 
the  Arabic  lute,  tuned  in  Fourths,  naturally  led 
to  this.  It  is  most  convenient  for  modem  habits 
of  thought  to  consider  the  series  as  one  of 
Fifths.  But  I  wish  to  draw  attention  to  the 
fact  that  in  all  probability  it  was  historically  a 
series  of  Fourths. — Translator,] 

t  [One  of  the  practical  results  of  the  Tonic 
Solfa  system  of  teaching  to  sing  the  diatonic 
major  scale  as  marked  on  p.  2746,  No.  i,  in  just 
intonation,  (see  App.  XVIII.,)  has  been  the 
discovery  that  it  is  not  so  easy  to  learn  to 
strike  the  proper  tone  by  a  knowledge  of  the 
interval  between  two  adjacent  tones  in  a 
melodic  passage,  as  by  a  knowledge  of  the 
mental  effect  produced  by  each  tone  of  the 
scale  in  relation  to  the  tonic.  These  mental 
effects  are  perhaps  not  veiy  clearly  character- 
ised by  the  mere  names  given  to  them  in  the 
Tonic  Solfa  books,  but  the  teacher  soon  makes 
his  class  understand  them,  and  then  finds 
them  the  most  valuable  instrument  which  he 
possesses  for  inspiring  a  feeling  for  just  intona- 
tion. On  these  characters  of  each  tone  in  the 
(just)  diatonic  scale,  a  system  of  manual  signs 
has  been  formed,  by  which  classes  are  con- 
stantly led.  Particulars  are  given  in  *  The 
Standard  Course  of  Lessons  and  Exercises  in 
the  Tonic  Solfa  Method  of  Teaching  Music, 
with  additional  exercises,  by  John  Curwen, 
new  edition,  re-written,  a.d.  1872.'  But  it 
may  be  convenient  to  mention  in  this  place 
the  characters  and  manual  signs  there  given 
{ib,  p.  iv.). 


I.  First  step. 

Do  J  Tonic,  *  the  strong  or  firm  tone,*  fist 
closed,  horizontal,  thumb  down. 

So,  Fifth, '  the  okand  or  bright  tone,*  the 
fingers  extended  and  horizontal,  hand  with 
little  finger  below  and  thumb  above,  so  that 
the  palm  of  the  hand  is  vertical.  ar 

Mi,  Major  Third,  *  the  stsadt  or  calm  tone/ 
fingers  extended  and  horizontal,  palm  of  hand 
horizontal  and  undermost. 

II.  Second  step. 

Re,  Second, '  the  bousino  or  hopefid  tone,* 
fingers  extended,  hand  forming  half  a  right 
angle  with  ground  pointing  upwards,  palm 
downwards. 

Ti,  Seventh,  *the  pdsbcino  or  sensitive 
tone,*  only  the  forefinger  extended  and  pointing 
up,  the  other  fingers  and  thumb  closed,  hand 
forming  half  a  right  angle  with  ground,  back 
of  hand  downwards. 

III.  Third  step. 

Fa,  Fourth,  *  the  desolate  or  awe-inspiring 
tone,*  only  the  forefinger  extended  and  point- 
ing down,  at  half  a  right  angle  with  the  ground, 
the  back  of  hand  upwards. 

La,  major  Sixth,  *  the  bad  or  weeping  tone,*  m 
fingers  fully  extended,  whole  hand  pointing 
down  with  a  weak  fall,  back  of  hand  upwards. 

It  is  thus  seen  that  the  order  of  teaching 
takes  the  tonic  chord  first,  then  the  dominant, 
and  lastly  the  subdominant.  The  doubtful 
Second  thus  comes  early  on.  *  The  teacher 
first  sings  the  exercise  to  [the  names  of]  con- 
secutive figures,  telling  his  pupils  that  he  is 
about  to  introduce  a  new  tone  (that  is  one  not 
DO,  n,  or  so),  and  asking  them  to  tell  him  on 
which  figure  it  falls.  When  they  have  distin- 
guished the  new  tone,  he  sings  the  exercise 
again — laa-ing  it  [this  is  calling  each  note  la] 
— and  asks  them  to  tell  him  how  that  tone 
'*  makes  them  feel.**  Those  who  can  describe 
the  feeling  hold  up  their  hands,  and  the 
teacher  asks  one  for  a  description.  But  others, 
who  are  not  satisfied  with  words,  may  also 
perceive  and  feci.    The  teacher  can  tell  by 


Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


28o  ARABIC  AND  PERSIAN  MUSICAL  SYSTEM.  pabt  m. 

Finally  there  is  no  perceptible  reason  in  the  series  of  Fifths  why  they  should 
not  be  carried  further,  after  the  gaps  in  the  diatonic  scale  have  been  supplied. 
Why  do  we  not  go  on  till  we  reach  the  chromatic  scale  of  Semitones  ?  To  what 
purpose  do  we  conclude  our  diatonic  scale  with  the  following  singularly  unequal 
arrangement  of  intervals — 

1,   I,  i,   I,   I,   I,  i 

The  new  tones  introduced  by  continuing  the  series  of  Fifths  would  lead  to  no  closer 
intervals  than  those  which  already  exist.  The  old  scale  of  five  tones  appears  to 
have  avoided  Semitones  as  being  too  close.  But  when  two  such  intervals  already 
appear  in  the  scale,  why  not  introduce  more  ? 

The  Arabic  and  Persian  musical  system^  so  far  as  its  nature  is  shewn  in  the 

writings  of  the  older  theorists,  also  knew  no  method  of  tuning  but  by  Fifths.   But 

^  this  system,  which  seems  to  have  developed  its  peculiarities  in  the  Persian  dynasty 

of  the  Sassanides  (a.d.  226-651)  before  the  Arabian  conquest,  shews  an  essential 

advance  on  the  Pythagorean  system  of  Fifths. 

In  order  to  judge  of  this  system  of  music,  which  has  been  hitherto  completely 
misunderstood,  the  following  relation  has  to  be  known.  By  tuning  four  Fifths 
upwards  from  G 

C±G±D±A±E 

we  come  to  a  tone,  E,  which  is  f  ^  or  a  comma  higher  than  the  natural  major 
Third  of  O,  which  we  write  Ei.  The  former  E  forms  the  major  Third  in  the 
Pythagorean  scale.    But  if  we  tune  eight  Fifths  downwards  from  C,  thus — 

C±F±JB\}±E\}±A\}±JD\)±G\}±Gj;}±F\} 

we  come  to  a  tone,  F\},  which  is  almost  exactly  the  same  as  the  natural  Ei*    The 
m  interval  of  C  to  F\}  is  expressed  by 

liH=f  x^ilMi  or  nearly  Jx|ff:f  [=384  cents]. 

their'  eyes  whether  they  have  done  so.    He  ^1,  B|.    We  can  partially  judge  of  them  by 

multiplies  examples  until  all  the  class  have  the  effects  of  equal  temperament,  which  melo- 

their  attention  fully  awakened  to  the  effect  of  dically  cannot    differ  much,    although    they 

the  new  tone.    This  done  he  tells  his  pupils  certainly  differ  sensibly,  from  those  of  Pytha- 

the  Sol-fa  name  and  the  manual  sign  for  the  gorean  intonation.    And  it  must  be  remem- 

new  tone,  and  guides  them  by  the  signs  to  bered  that  singers  actually  learn  to  sing  in 

Sol-fa  the  exercise  and  tiiemselves  produce  the  equal  temperament,  in  which  all  major  Thirds 

proper  effect.     The  signs  are  better  in  this  are   14  cents  too  sharp,  and  then  find  just 

case    than  the  notation,  because  with  them  major  Thirds  intolerably  flat !  To  this  I  would 

the  teacher  can  best  command  the  attention  add  the  following  anecdote  quoted  from  F^tis 

of   every    eye  and  ear    and   voice,    and    at  {Hist.  G&rUrale  de  la  Musique,  vol.  ii.  p.  27)  by 

the   first  introduction    of    a  tone,  attention  Prof.  Land  (Oamme  Arabe,  p.   19  footnote), 

should  be  acute '  (ibid,  p.  15).    This  passage,  containing  '  a  fact,'  as  he  says,  *  which  could 

the  result  of  practice  with  hundreds  of  thou-  not  be  believed,  if  it  were  not  attested  by  the 

sands  of  chilchren,  shews  that  a  totally  new  person  whom  it  concerns.     The  celebrated 

^  principle  of  understanding  the  relation  of  the  organist   M.  Lemmens,  who  was    bom  in  a 

tones  in  a  scale  to  the  tonic  has  not  only  been  village  of  Gampine  [or  Eempenland,  a  district 

introduced,  but  worked  out  on  a  large  scale  in  the  Belgian  province  of  Limbourg,  5i°i5'  N. 

practically,and,a8lmyBelf  know,  successfully,  lat.,  5^20^  £.  long.],  studied  music  in  early 

See  Prof.  Helmholtz's  own  impression  of  the  youth  upon  a  clavecin  (harpsichord),  which 

success,  as  long  ago  as  1864,  in  App.  XVIII.  had  been  long  dreadfully  out  of  tune,  because 

Since  that  time  great   experience  has  been  no  tuner  existed  in  the  district.    Fortunately* 

gained  and  many  methods  improved.    But  the  an  organ-builder  was  summoned  to  repair  the 

object  of  introducing  this  notice  here  is  to  organ  at  the  abbey  of  Everbode  near  that 

shew  that  proper  training  (such  as  the  ancient  village.    By  chance  he  called  upon  the  young 

Greeks  certainly  had)  could  produce  the  corre-  musician's  father,  and  heard  the  boy  play  on 

Bjpondjngfeeling  for  the  effect  of  any  tone  in  any  his  miserable  instrument.      Shocked  at  the 

scale  anyhow  divided,  independently  of  the  rela-  multitude  of  false  notes  which  struck  his  ear, 

tion  of  consonances ,  and  that  this  consideration  he  immediately  determined  to  tune  the  clavecin, 

may  help  to  explain  the  persistence  of  many  When  he  had  done  so,  M.  Lemmens  experi- 

scales  which    are  harmonically  inexplicable,  enced  the  most  disagreeable  sensations,  and  it 

No  doubt  Pythagorean  singers  hit  the  degrees  was  some  time  before  he  could  habituate  his 

of  their  scale  quite  correctly,  and  no  doubt  the  ear  to  the  correct  intervals,  having  been  bo 

'  mental  effects  '  of  their  A^  E^  B,  were  very  long  misled  by  different  relations.'    Uenoe, 

different  from  those  of  the  harmonisable  ^p  false  intervals  may  seem  natural. — TransUUorJ] 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


CHAP.  XIV.       AKABIC  AND  PEESIAN  MUSICAL  SYSTEM. 


281 


Hence  the  tone  F\}  is  lower  than  the  natural  major  Third  ^1  [=386  cents]  by 
the  extremely  small  interval  ffj  [=2  cents],  which  is  about  the  eleventh  part  of  a 
comma  [=22  cents].  This  interval  between  F\}  and  ^1  is  practically  scarcely  per- 
ceptible, or  at  most  only  perceptible  by  the  extremely  slow  beats  produced  by  the 
chord  C...F\}..,G  [=C  384  jP^  318  G]  upon  an  instrument  most  exactly  tuned. 
Practically,  then,  we  may  without  hesitation  assume  that  the  two  tones  Fi^  and  E^ 
are  identical,  and  of  course  that  their  Fifths  are  also  identical,  or 

F\}  =  ^1,  q?  =  -Bi,  Gb  =  FS,  &c.* 

Now  in  the  Arabic  and  Persian  scale  the  Octave  is  divided  into  16  intervals,  but 
in  our  equal  temperament  it  is  divided  into  6  whole  Tones.  Modern  [European] 
interpreters  of  the  Arabic  and  Persian  system  of  music  have  hence  been  misled 
into  the  conclusion  that  each  of  the  17  degrees  of  the  scale  corresponded  to  about 
the  third  of  a  Tone  in  our  music.  In  that  case  the  intonation  of  the  degrees  in  IT 
the  Arabic  and  Persian  scale  would  not  be  executable  on  our  instruments.  But 
in  Eiesewetter's  work  on  the  Music  of  the  Arabs,t  which  was  written  with  the 


*  [On  this  substitation,  which  amounts  to 
a  temperament  with  perfect  Fifths,  and  major 
Thirds  too  flat  by  a  skhisma,  or  nearly  the 
eleventh  of  a  comma,  and  which  I  therefore 
call  skhismic  temperament,  see  Appendix  XX. 
section  A.  art.  1 7.  It  is  convenient  to  use  a  grave 
accent  prefixed  thas,  ^^p  to  show  flattening  by 
a  skhisma,  and  to  read  it  as  skhismic,  thus, 

*  skhismic  E  one.'  The  above  equations  can 
therefore  be  made  precise  by  writing  ^b»^JS7i, 
Cb«'B„  Gb  =  '^,»  ,  &o.— Translator.] 

t  R.  G.  Kiesewetter,  Die  Musik  der  Araher 
nach  Originalquellen  dargestellt,  mit  einem 
Vorworte  von  dem  Freiherm  von  Hammer- 
PurgstalU  Leipzig,  1842,  pp.  32,  33.  The 
directions  given  in  an  anonymous  manuscript 
of  the  666th  year  of  the  Hegira,  a.d.  1267,  in 
the  possession  of  Prof.  Salisbury  [of  Yale  Coll.], 
are  essentially  the  same.  See  Journal  of  the 
American  Oriental  Society,  vol.  i.  p.  1 74.  [Since 
the  publication  of  the  4th  German  edition 
of  this  work  in  1877,  the  whole  history  of  the 
Arabic  scale  has  been  reinvestigated  from  the 
original  Arabic  sources  by  Herr  J.  P.  N.  Land, 
D.B.,  Professor  of  Mental  Philosophy  at 
Leyden,  an  Oriental  scholar  and  a  musician, 
and  the  results  were  published  first  in  Dutch 
as  a  paper  in  the  Transactions  of  the  Dutch 
Academy  of  Sciences,  division  Literature,  2nd 
series,  vol.  ix.,  and  separately  under  the  title 
of  Over  de  Toonladders  der  Arabische  Mvaiek 
(On  the  Scales  of  Arabic  Music)  in  1880,  and 
secondly  in  French  as  a  paper  conmiunicated 
to  the  International  Congress  of  Orientalists  at 
Leyden  in  1882  and  pubUshed  in  vol.  ii.  of  their 

*  Transactions,'  and  also  separately  in  1884  as 
liecherches  sur  Vhistoire  de  la  Garrnne  Arabe, 
This  paper  supersedes  in  many  respects  the 
work  of  Kiesewetter  and  von  Hammer-Furgstall, 
of  whom  the  first  was  a  musician  but  not  an 
Orientalist,  and  the  second  an  Orientalist  but  not 
a  musician.  Alf  arables  scale  was  produced  by  a 
succession  of  Fifths  [or  rather  Fourths,  see 
p.  42,  note],  but  a  century  and  a  half  previously 
Zalzal  had  introduced  a  new  interval  22  :  27  » 
355  cents,  which  Prof.  Land  terms  a  neutral 
Third.  It  is  actually  if  x  §  or  151  +  204  cents, 
that  is,  three  quarters  of  a  Tone  sharper  than 
a  major  tone,  whereas  the  major  Third  is  182 
cents  or  a  minor  Tone  sharper,  and  the  minor 
Third  was  only  a  diatonic  Semitone  112  cents 
sharper.  The  interval  12  :  11 » 151  cents  is  the 


well-known  trumpet  interval  between  the  shar- 
pened Fourth  and  Fifth,  the  nth  and  12th  har- 
monics, as  may  be  heard  in  the  fifth  Octave  of 
the  Harmonical  *'/"  :  g"'.  This  on  the  Arabic 
lute  was  necessarily  accompanied  by  a  similar 
interval  on  the  next  string,  498 -(-3 55^853 
cents.  These  two  notes  eventually  superseded 
the  old  Pythagorean  minor  Third  of  294  cents 
and  the  Fourth  above  it  of  792  cents;  and 
seem  entirely  out  of  the  reach  of  a  succession 
of  Fifths  or  Fourths.  But  it  was  the  object  of 
Abdulqadir  and  others  to  form  a  succession  of 
Fifths  (or  rather  Fourths)  which  would  include 
these  two  intervals,  at  least  approximately,  f 
This  they  accomplished  within  less  than  30  cents 
by  their  384  and  882  cents.  It  does  not  appear 
to  have  been  Abdulqadir's  object  to  approxi- 
mate to  the  just  major  Third  386,  and  just 
major  Sixth  884,  but  to  get  by  means  of  Fifths 
or  Fourths  certain  tones  which  would  pass  as 
Zalzal 's.  The  list  in  the  text  (p.  2826)  gives 
the  seventeen  tones  thus  produced  with  the  in- 
tervals that  they  form  with  each  other,  and  Prof. 
Helmholtz's  names  of  the  notes,  completed  by 
a  grave  accent.  Here  I  re-arrange  them  in 
order  of  Fifths  down  or  Fourths  up,  the  ap- 
proximate Thirds  being  added  immediately  to 
the  right,  and  the  numbers  showing  the  interval 
in  cents  from  C : 

E  408 

A    906,  D\>    ='Ci5    90 

D  204,  Ob    «-'^,«  588 

Q  702,  Cb    «'^i  1086  ^ 

C       o,  F\>    -i'^i    384 

F  498,  Bbb^'il,    882 

jBb996,  i;bb«='A    'So 

£b294,  ilbb='Oi    678 

Ab192,  Dbb  =  'Ci  1176 

Observe  that  the  real  major  Third  was  the 
Pythagorean  408  cents,  as  the  minor  Third 
was  the  Pythagorean  294  cents.  Also  that 
180  cents  was  within  two  cents  of  the  minor 
tone  182  cents.  But  these  approximations 
were  probably  not  contemplated. 

An  English  concertina,  which  has  fourteen 
notes  to  the  Octave,  was  tuned  with  thirteen 
consecutive  Fifths  from  Gb  to  CS,  so  that  I 
was  able  to  try  the  chords  ADbE  DObA, 
that  is,  A^CJ^E,  D'F^ZA,  where  the  major 
Thirds  are  two  cents  too  flat,  and  compare 
them  with  the  Pythagorean  chords  ACtEt 


Digitized  by  V^OOQIC 


282  ABABIC  AND  PEESIAN  MUSICAL  SYSTEM.         pabt  ru. 

assistance  of  the  celebrated  Orientalist  von  Hammer-Purgstall,  there  is  given  a 
translation  of  the  directions  for  the  division  of  the  monoohord  laid  down  by  Abdul 
Kadir,  a  celebrated  Persian  theorist  of  the  fourteenth  century  of  oar  era,  that  lived 
at  the  courts  of  Timur  and  Bajazet.  These  directions  enable  us  to  calculate  the 
intonation  of  the  Oriental  scale  with  perfect  certainty.  These  directions  also  agree 
in  essentials  both  with  those  of  the  much  older  Farabi,*  (who  died  in  a.d.  950),  and 
of  his  own  contemporary,  Mahmud  Shirazi,t  (who  died  in  1315),  for  dividing  the 
fingerboard  of  lutes.  According  to  the  directions  of  Abdul  Eadir  all  the  tonal 
degrees  of  the  Arabic  scale  are  obtained  by  a  series  of  16  Fifths,  and  if  we  call  the 
lowest  degree  C,  and  arrange  them  in  order  of  pitch  within  the  compass  of  an 
Octave,  they  will  be  the  following,  as  expressed  in  our  notation  [with  the  addition 
of  the  grave  accent  explained  in  p.  281&,  note  *]. 

I)    C  -  2)  Db-  3)  'D,^4)  D^  s)^>-  6)  'E,^ 
IT  7)  ^-  8)  i^  -  9)   Gb-io)^Gi--ii)  G    -12)  ^b- 

13)  ^ili— 14)  A  -is)  B[}  -16)  ^^1-17)  "ci  ^iS)  c 

where  the  line— between  two  tones  indicates  the  interval  of  a  Pythagorean  Linmia 
fi7  (which  is  nearly  f^  [=90  cents]),  and  the  sign  -^  a  Pythagorean  comma 
[=531441  :  524288=24  cents].  The  Limma  is  about  i  and  the  Pythagorean 
comma  a  little  more  than  ^  and  less  than  f  of  the  natural  Semitone  \^  [=112 
cents]. 

Abdul  Eadir  assigns  the  following  intonation  to  the  three  first  of  the  12  prin- 
cipal tonal  modes  or  Makamiat : — 

Arabic  Ancient  Qreek 

1.  Uschak  C...D  ,..E  ..,F...O  ...4  ...J5l>...c  Hypophrygian  or  Ionic. 

2.  Newa     C..D   ...Ei^.„F.,.G  ...-4b...J5b...c  Hypodorian  or  Eolio. 

-r       3.  Buselik  C...I)t>...j&b-- -^•••G^l>--^b--^-c  Mixolydian,  [allonp.  269a.] 

These  three  are  therefore  completely  identical  with  the  ancient  Greek  scales  in 
Pythagorean  intonation.^:  Since  the  Arabic  theoreticians  divide  these  scales  into 
the  Fourth  G...F  and  the  Fifth  F±,c,  and  since  O,  F  and  B\}  are  considered  to  be 
invariable  tones,  and  the  others  to  be  variable,  it  is  probable  that  F  must  be 
regarded  as  the  tonic.    In  this  case 

1.  Uschak  would  be  =  i^  major. 

2.  Newa  would  be    =  the  mode  of  the  minor  Seventh  of  i^.§ 

3.  Buselik  would  be  =  the  mode  of  the  minor  Sixth  of  F. 

all  three  in  Pythagorean  intonation.  The  Persian  school  also  considers  the  scales 
to  be  related. 

DFt  A,    The  latter  were  offensive,  the  former  their  dftssical  lute,  to  which  alone  the  above 

^  indistiiiguishable    from    just.      It  seems    re-  refers. — Translator.] 

markable,  therefore,  how  with  suoh  a  collection  *  J.  G.  L.  Eosegarten,  Alii  IspaJutnensiB 

of  notes  the  Arabs  escaped  harmonic  music.  Zdber  CaiUUenartun^  pp.  76-86. 

But  it  will  be  seen  on  examining  the  scales  f  Eiesewetter,  Die  Mtuik  der  Amber  nach 

formed  from  them  (see  espeoia^y  p.  284^,  note),  Originalquellen  darg.,  p.  33. 

that  they  were  perfectly  unadapted  for  har-  \  [Not  therefore  according  to  the  forms  on 

mony,  which  would  have  occasioned  a  perfect  p.  26&i',  note,  but  on  the  more  recent  Pytha- 

revolution  in  their  musical  systems.  gorean  imitation  of  those  forms.    They  are 

There  was  certainly  no  attempt  to  divide  respectively  the  representatives  of  scales  2,  4, 

the  scale  as  Villoteau  supposed  into  seventeen  and  6  of  that  note. — Translator,'] 
equal  parts  each  of  about  70*6  cents,  for  no  §  [In  the  German  text,  Quartengsscklecht^ 

such  intervals  occur,  still  less  any  third  parts  or  the  mode  of  the  Fourth  of  F.    The  tones 

of  a  (tempered)  tone  of  66f  cents,  which  was  a  in  the  mode  of  the  Fourth  of  ^  are  those  in 

mere  hallucination  of  Yilloteau's.  the  Pythagorean  scale  of  Bb ,  or,  in  order  of 

This  system  of  Abdulqadir  prevailed  from  Fifths,  E\f  ±B\>±F±C±Q±B±At  and  the 

the  thirteenth  to  the  fifteenth  century.    The  tones  of  the  mode  of  the  minor  Seventh  of  F 

modem  division  into  twenty-four  Quartertones  are  those  in  the  Pythagorean  scale  otE\>%  or,  in 

is  noticed  on  p.  2646  and  note  **.  order  of  Fifths,  A\) ±E\> ± B>> ±F ± C ±0 ±  D. 

The   Arabs,  however,  had    also    entirely  The  correction  is  therefore  evident. — Trans- 

dilterent  scales  for  other  inbtruments  than  lalor,] 

Digitized  by  VjOOQIC 


CHAP.  XIV.       ARABIC  AND  PERSIAN  MUSICAL  SYSTEM.  283 

The  next  group  consists  of  five  tonal  modes  having  just  or  natural  intonation, 
namely : 


4.  Rast 0  ...  ^D,  ...  'E, ...  F  ...  G  ...  'A^  ...  JB\} 

5.  Husseini.    .    .    .  C  ... 'Di  „.  Ej[}  ...  F  ...'G^...  A\}  ...  B\} 

6.  Hidschaf.    .     .    .  C  ...'Dy  ...  E\}  ...  F  ...'G^...'!^  ...  J^ 

7.  Rahewi   •    .    .    .  C  ... 'D^  ...'F^  ...  F  ...'Gi...  A\)  ...  B\} 

8.  Sengule  .    .    .    .  G  ...  D   ...'E^  ...  F  ...'G^... 'A^  ...  B\} 


..  c 

..  c 

..  G 

..  C 

..  c 


Bast  may  be  regarded  as  the  mode  of  the  Fourth  of  C ;  Hidschaf  as  the  mode 
of  the  Fourth  of  F ;  Husseini  as  the  mode  of  the  Fourth  oi  B^\  as  such  they 
would  have  perfectly  natural  intonation.  In  Bahetvi,  if  we  refer  it  to  the  tonic  F, 
the  minor  third  A\}  is  in  Pythagorean,  not  natural,  intonation.  It  might  be  re- 
garded as  the  mode  of  the  minor  Seventh  of  i^  in  which  the  major  Seventh  E^  is  m 
used  as  the  leading  note  in  place  of  the  minor  Seventh,  as  in  our  own  minor  mode. 
The  natural  intonation  of  such  a  tonal  mode  cannot,  indeed,  be  properly  repre- 
sented by  the  existing  17  tonal  degrees.  It  becomes  necessary  to  take  either 
Pythagorean  minor  Thirds  and  natural  major  Thirds  or  conversely.  Husseini  may 
be  regarded  as  the  same  tonal  mode  with  Bahetvi,  having  the  same  false  minor 
Third,  but  a  minor  Seventh.  Finally  Sengule  may  be  regarded  as  F  major  with  a 
Pythagorean  Sixth.  Bast  may  be  conceived  in  the  same  way ;  they  are  merely  dis- 
tinguished by  the  different  values  of  the  Seconds  G  oi^Gi. 

The  four  last  Makamat  have  each  8  tones,  new  intercalary  tones  being  em- 
ployed. Two  of  them  resemble  the  modes  Bast  and  Sengule,  and  between  B\} 
and  C  there  is  an  intercalary  tone  ^Cy  introduced ;  named 

9.  Irak     .    .    .    C...'D,...'Ey...F...G  ...'A,...B\}...'c,..x 

10.  Iszfahan   .    .    G...D  ...'E^...  F..:Gy..:Ai...  B\}..:cy...c  ^ 

The  last  transposed  a  Fourth  gives 

11.  Biisiirg     .    •    C...  D  ...'Ey...  F...'Gi...G  ...  A  ...'By...c 
The  last  tonal  mode  is 

12.  Zirefkend.    .    C...'Dy...E\}...F..:Gy...A\}...''Ay...'By...c 

which  certainly,  if  rightly  reported,  is  a  very  singular  creation.  It  might  be 
looked  upon  as  a  minor  scale  with  a  major  Seventh,  and  both  a  major  and  minor 
Sixth,  but  then  the  Fifth  ^Gx  is  wrong.  On  the  other  hand,  if  F  is  taken  as  the 
tonic,  it  has  no  Fourth,  for  which  certainly  there  is  some  analogy  in  the  Mixo- 
lydian  and  Hypolydian  scales.  The  instructions  for  scales  of  eight  notes  are  very 
contradictory,  to  judge  by  the  different  authorities  cited  by  Kiesewetter. 

The  following  four  are  distinguished  as  the  principal  modes  of  the  Makamat : —  ^ 

1.  Uschak    =  Pythagorean  F  major. 

2.  Rast        =  Natural  mode  of  the  Fourth  of  (7,  or  natural  F  major  with  acute 

Sixth. 

3.  Husseini  =  Natural  mode  of  the  minor  Seventh  of  F. 

4.  Hidschaf  =  Natural  mode  of  the  Fourth  of  F. 

We  find,  then,  a  decided  predominance  of  scales  with  a  perfectly  correct  natural  in- 
tonation, which  has  been  attained  by  a  skilful  use  of  a  continued  series  of  Fifths. 
This  makes  the  Arabic  and  Persian  tonal  system  very  noteworthy  in  the  history  of 
the  development  of  music.  Moreover,  in  some  of  these  scales  we  find  ascending 
leading  notes,  which  are  perfectly  foreign  to  the  Greek  scales.  Thus  in  Rahewi, 
E\  is  the  leading  note  to  F,  although  the  minor  Third -4|>  stands  above  F,  while  no 
Greek  scale  could  have  allowed  this  without  at  the  same  time  changing  ^^  into  E\}, 

Digitized  by  V^OOQIC 


284 


ARABIC  AND  PERSIAN  MUSICAL  SYSTEM. 


PABT  lU. 


Similarly  in  Zirefkend  the  Bi  is  used  as  a  leading  note  to  C,  although  the  minor 
Third  E\}  is  used  above  0.* 

♦  [Prof.  Land  (Gamme  Arabe^  p.  38,  note  3) 
says  *  some  of  the  descriptions  of  Prof.  Helm- 
holtz,  borrowed  from  Kiesewetter,  do  not  quite 
correspond  with  the  original  data.'  It  will  be 
interesting  therefore  to  give  these  scales  as  Prof. 
Land  describes  them  with  his  (more  exact) 
French  orthography  of  the  Arabic  names  and 
in  his  order.  The  notation  is  the  Translator's, 
M|  being  24  cents  flatter  than  A. 

1.  'OcJiaq,  Our  F  major  commencing  (as 
shewn  by  [)  with  the  dominant,  FOABb 
[CDEF.  *  This  commencement  is  the  inevitable 
consequence  of  the  progression  by  conjunct 
tetrachords  which  belongs  to  the  lute.    'Ochaq 

^  is  as  it  were  the  type  of  all  these  maqdmdty  the 
others  of  which  differ  at  one  time  like  the 
tropes  or  modes  of  the  Greeks  and  of  the 
middle  ages,  by  the  displacement  of  both  the 
Bemitones  at  once,  and  at  other  times  like  the 
Greek  genera,  by  exchanges  of  intervals  with- 
out disturbing  the  scheme  of  two  conjunct 
tetrachords  followed  by  a  tone,  with  the  ex- 
ception of  Nos.  7  and  8,  which  are  more  distinct 
from  the  model  maqd?na.* 

2.  Nawd,  '  We  may  say  that  the  scale  is 
that  -of  E\>  major,  beginning  with  the  Sixth.' 
EbFQABb[CDE\>, 

3.  B<msiUk  or  Ahou-sillk,  'The  scale  of 
D\>  major  beginning  at  the  Seventh,  DbE\> 
FGb  AbBb  [CDb.'  The  Pythagorean  into- 
nation of  the  three  first  scales  renders  them 
non-harmonic. 

%  4.  Bast,  '  The  same  as  'Ochaq  except  that 
the  Third  A  and  the  Seventh  E  are  depressed 
by  a  Pyth.  comma,  FQ'A^Bb  [CD'E^F,  which 
makes  them  just  rather  than  Pythagorean.' 
The  subdominant  BbDF  is  non-harmonic. 

5.  ^Iraq.  *  Like  Bast,  but  with  the  second 
and  the  sixth  above  diminished  by  a  Pyth. 
comma,  which  makes  the  second  nearly  the 
minor  Second  10  :  9,  and  with  grave  supple- 
mentary Fifth.'  F'O,  'A^Bb  ^C,  [  C 'D,  'E,  F. 
Tuis  has  the  proper  subdominant  Bb^D^F,  but 
the  double  Fifth  is  quite  non-harmonic. 

6.  I^ahdn.  ^Bast  enriched  with  a  grave 
supplementary  Fifth.'  FG'A^Bb'C^[CD'E^F. 
Here  both  the  subdominant  BbDF  and  double 
Fifth  render  the  scale  non-harmonic. 

7.  Zirafkend,  C 'D,  EbF'G^Ab  M,  ^J5,  C. 
*  An  artificial  scale  composed  of  fragments  of 
those  of    Eb   (eb/ Vi<*t>  c  *d,  fib,   Third  and 

^  Seventh  almost  just)  and  of  C  (c  ^d^f^a^  ^5,  c, 
Second  minor  and  Sixth  nearly  just)  varied 
also  with  Pythagorean  A  ot  D  and  ^^p'  Of 
course  entirely  non -harmonic. 

8.  Bousourk,  *  C  major  with  the  Second, 
Third,  and  Seventh  diminished  by  a  Pyth. 
comma,  and  with  a  grave  supplementary  Fifth.' 
C D;E^FG^GA'Bfi.  Both  subdominant  and 
dominant  are  non -harmonic. 

9.  Zetikouleh.  *  Differs  from  Bast  only  in 
having  the  Second  minor.'  F^'Gi^AiBbiC 
D'E^F,    Subdominant  non-harmonic. 

10.  Bdhawi.  *F  minor  commencing  with 
the  Fifth,  but  with  the  Sixth  and  Seventh 
each  increased  by  a  Limma  =  90  cents,  and  the 
Second  diminished  by  a  Pythagorean  comma, 
very  nearly  our  just  ascending  scale  of  F 
minor.'  F'GiAbBblC'D^E.F.  The  Pyth. 
scale  of  F  minor  is  FGAbBb[C DbEbF. 
Here  Db=^90  cents;    90  +  90=180^204-24 


cents -^Dp  24  cents  being  the  Pyth.  comma. 
Similarly  J?b  «  294  cents ;  294  +  90  =  384 « 408 
—  24  cents  =  ^E^.■  Entirely  non-harmonio. 

11.  HhosaKni,  *Like  Nawa,  but  with  the 
Third  and  Seventh  diminished  by  a  Pyth. 
comma.'  EbF^G^ABb [C'D^ Eb.  Entirely 
non-harmonic. 

12.  Hhidjdzi,  *Bb  major,  beginning  with 
the  Second  and  with  the  Third,  Sixt£,  and 
Seventh  diminished  and  therefore  nearly  just.' 
Bb[C'D^EbIf''G^'A^Bb.  This  is  the  only  one 
of  tiiese  scales  which  is  practically  harmonic. 

If  we  restore  the  proper  names  of  the  notes 
in  the  series  of  Fifths  or  Fourths,  (as  in  p.  28  id') 
calculate  the  cents  between  each  pair  of  notes 
and  from  the  first  to  each  note,  and  begin  with 
the  note  indicated,  we  shall  have  a  better  idea 
of  the  real  nature  of  these  scales,  thus : 

1.  'Ochaq,  C204  D  20^  E  ^  F  21^  G204 

o  sn4  408  498  7« 

il  90  JBb  204  C 

906  996  xaoo 

2.  Nawd.  C  204  D  90  £b  204  F  204  O  204 

o  904  394  498  70a 

A^  Bb  204  C 

906  99^  1200 

3.  BousilXk.  C  90    Db  204    Eb  204    F   90 

o  90  394  49B 

Gb  204  Ab  204  Bb  204  C 

4.  Bast,  C  204   D  180  Fb  114  F  20^  Q  i8q 

O  904  384  498  TQB 

Bbb  114  Bb204  C 

88b  996  X900 

5.  'Irdq,  C  180    ^bb  204    Fb    114  F    180 

o  i8b  384  498 

^bb  204  Bbb  114  Bb  180   Dbb  24  C. 

678  88b  996  XX76  laoo 

This  double  initial Z>bb,C  maybe  compared  to 
our  double  second  in  just  major  scales,  and  pos- 
sibly has  to  be  explained  in  the  same  way  as  a 
real  modulation. 

6.  I^ahdn.  O  180   Ebb  20^   Fb  iiA  ^  204 

o  180  3B4  49« 

G  180  Bbb  114  Bb  180  Dbb  24  G 

70a  88b  996  X176  xaoo 

7.  Zirafkend.  C  180  £bbii4  £b204  F  180 

o  180  994  49B 

Abb  114  Ab  90  Bbb  204  Cb  114  C 

678  79a  SSa  1086  xaoo 

8.  Bomourk,  C  180  Ebb  204  ^b  114  F  180 

o  x8o  3B4  498 

Abb  24  G  204  A  iSo  Cb  114  O 

678  70a  906  1086  xaoo 

9.  Zenkouleh.  C  204  B  180  ^b  114  F   180 

o  ao4  384  498 

Abb  204  Bbb  114  Bb  204  C 

678  88a  996  xaoo 

io»  Bdhawi.  C  180  Ebb  204  ^b  114   F  180 
0x80  384  49B 

Abb  114  Ab  204  Bb  204  C 

678  79a  996  laoo 

II, IDvosaXni.  C  iSo  Ebb  114  i?b204  J^  180 
Abb  228  il  90  Bb  204  G 

678  906  996  xaoo 

12.  Hhidjazi,  C  180  Ebb  114  J5b  204  F  180 

o  180  994  498 

^bb  204  Bbb  114  Bb  204  C 

678  883  996  xaoo 

Of  these  I  have  been  able  to  play  i,  2,  3  direct, 
and  4, 5, 10, 12  by  transposition  upon  my  Pytha- 
gorean concertina  (p.  28 id').  When  12  beKHis 
with  Bb,  or  is  played  by  transposition  abd'\> 
d'cfj'ba'b  a't  it  is  indistinguishable  from  the 


Digitized  by^OOQlC 


CHAP.  XIV.  MEANING  OP  THE  LEADING  NOTE.  285 

At  a  little  later  period  a  new  musical  system  was  developed  in  Persia  with 
12  Semitones  to  the  Octave,  analogous  to  the  modem  European  system.  Kiese- 
wetter  here  hazards  the  very  unlikely  hypothesis  that  this  scale  was  introduced 
into  Persia  hy  Christian  missionaries.  But  it  is  clear  that  the  system  of  17  tonal 
degrees  which  had  been  previously  in  popular  use,  merely  required  the  feehng  for  the 
finer  distinctions  to  grow  dull  so  that  intervals  which  differed  only  by  a  [Pythagorean] 
comma  should  be  confused,  in  order  to  generate  the  system  of  12  Semitones.*  No 
foreign  influence  was  necessary  here.  Moreover,  the  Greek  system  of  music  had 
long  been  taught  to  the  Arabs  and  Persians  by  Alf&rabl.  Again,  the  European 
theory  of  music  had  not  made  any  essential  advance  in  the  fourteenth  and  fifteenth 
centuries,  if  we  except  the  study  of  harmony,  which  never  found  favour  with  the 
Orientals.  Hence  the  Europeans  of  those  days  could  teach  the  Orientals  nothing 
that  they  did  not  already  know  better  themselves,  except  some  imperfect  rudiments 
of  harmony  which  they  did  not  want.  There  is  much  more  reason,  I  think,  for  ^ 
asking  whether  the  imperfect  fragments  of  the  natural  system  which  we  find 
among  the  Alexandrine  Greeks,  do  not  depend  on  Persian  traditions,  and  also, 
whether  the  Europeans  in  the  time  of  the  Crusades  did  not  learn  much  music  from 
the  Orientals.  It  is  very  probable  that  they  brought  tlie  lute-shaped  instruments 
with  fingerboards  and  the  bowed  instruments  from  the  East.  In  the  construction 
of  tonal  modes  we  might  especially  instance  the  use  of  the  leading  note,  which 
we  have  here  found  existing  in  the  East,  and  which  at  that  time  also  began  to  figure 
in  the  Western  music. 

The  use  of  the  major  Seventh  of  the  scale  as  a  leading  note  to  the  tonic  marks 
a  new  conception,  which  admitted  of  being  used  for  the  further  development  of 
the  tonal  degrees  of  a  scale,  even  within  the  domain  of  purely  homophonic  music. 
The  tone  Bi  in  the  major  scale  of  C  has  the  most  distant  relationship  of  all  the 
tones  to  the  tonic  0,  because  as  the  major  Third  of  the  dominant  G,  it  has  a  less 
close  connection  with  it  than  its  Fifth  X>.  We  may  perhaps  assume  this  to  be  the  IT 
reason  why,  when  a  sixth  tone  was  introduced  into  some  Gaehc  airs,  the  Seventh 
was  usually  omitted.  But,  on  the  other  hand,  the  major  Seventh  Bi  developed  a 
peculiar  relation  to  the  tonic,  which  in  modem  music  is  indicated  by  caUing  it 
the  leading  7U>te.  The  major  Seventh  B^  differs  from  the  Octave  c  of  the  tonic  by 
the  smallest  interval  in  the  scale,  namely  a  Semitone,  and  this  proximity  to  the 
tonic  allows  the  Seventh  to  be  struck  easily  and  pretty  surely,  even  when  starting 
from  tones  in  the  scale  which  are  not  at  all  related  to  jBj.  The  leap  F...B1 
[=45  '  32=590  cents],  for  example,  is  difficult,  because  there  is  no  relationship  at 
all  between  the  tones.  But  when  a  singer  has  to  perform  the  passage  F...Bi...c, 
he  conceives  the  interval  F...C,  which  he  can  easily  execute,  but  does  not  force  his 
voice  up  sufficiently  high  to  reach  c  at  first,  and  thus  strikes  Bi  on  the  way.  Thus 
Bi  assumes  the  appearance  of  a  preparation  for  c,  and  this  view  alone  justifies  it 
to  the  ears  of  a  hstener,  by  whom  the  transition  into  c  is,  therefore,  expected. 
Hence  it  has  been  said  that  Bi  leads  to  c ;  or  that  Bi  is  the  leading  note  to  c.    In  f 

jast  «oale   abCi%d'e'f/tg/Za',    The   three  melodies    the  Orientals,  as  we  do,  selected 

chords   d^g'ba't  ad'\>e\  ef^bh  are  perfectly  from  them  several  series  of  7  [occasionally  8] 

good,  and  the  passage  d'b  0V,  d^b  a',  e'tt'ii^'b,  tones,  very  slightly  different  from  our  dia- 

e^a'b  b't  d! b  tla!  perfectly  good,  much  better  than  tonic  scales.*    But  so  materially  different  that 

on  the  piano.    Yet  it  never  occurred  to  Arabs  any  attempt  to  play  harmonies  upon  them 

to  play  in  harmony.  would  result  in  frightful  dissonance. — Traws- 

*  In  face  of  these  historical  scales,'  observes  latorJ] 
Prof.  Land  (t&u2.p.38),  *  it  is  difiScult  to  conceive  *  [If  we  suppose  the  pairs  of  notes  in  (  ) 
how  Eiesewetter  could  say  that  the  17  degrees  of  to  have  been  confused  into  one  by  neglecting 
the  complete  scale  were  not  treated  like  sharps  the  Pythagorean  comma,  then  the  series  of 
and  flats,  but  that  each  one  had  tiie  same  notes  on  p.  2826  becomes  C  Db  CA  D)  Eb  {'E^ 
importance.  On  the  contrary,  the  17  degrees  E)  F  Gb  fO^O)  Ab  ^A^A)  Bb  'B,  i^CiC), 
were  like  our  12  Semitones  to  the  Octave,  or,  wnence  the  equally  tempered  scale  C  Db  D 
still  better,  like  the  17  intervals  of  the  so-  Eb  E  F  Ob  G  Ab  A  Bb  B  c  immediately 
called  enharmonic  scale,  which  distinguishes  follows.  In  Meshaqah's  scale  of  24  Quarter- 
sharps  and  flats,  without  dividing  the  Semi-  tones,  p.  264c,  that  of  12  Semitones  is  also 
tones  E  to  Ff  and  B  to  C    To  compose  their  implicitly  contained.— Traru^a^.] 

Digitized  by^OOQlC 


286  MEANING  OF  THE  LEADING  NOTE;  part  in. 

this  sense  it  also  becomes  easy  to  sharpen  Bi  somewhat,  making  it  B,  for  example, 
to  bring  it  near  to  c,  and  mark  its  reference  to  that  tone  more  distinctly. 

According  to  my  own  feeling,  the  leading  effect  of  the  tone  ^i  is  much  more 
marked  in  such  passages  as  i^...jBi...c  otF-\-Ax..,Bi,.,c,  in  which  ^i  is  not  related 
to  the  preceding  tones,  than  in  such  a  passage  as  0+Bi.,.c  where  it  is.  But  as  I 
have  found  nothing  on  this  point  in  musical  writings,  I  do  not  know  whether 
musicians  are  likely  to  agree  with  me  in  this  opinion.  For  the  other  Semitone  of 
the  scale,  Ei.,.F,  the  Ei  does  not  seem  to  lead  to  F,  if  the  tonality  of  the  melody 
is  well  preserved,  because  in  this  case  Ei  has  its  own  independent  relation  to  the 
tonic,  and  hence  is  musically  quite  determinate.  The  hearer,  then,  has  no 
occasion  to  regard  Ei  as  a  mere  preparation  for  F.  Similarly  for  the  interval 
G..,A^\}  [=112  cents]  in  the  minor  mode.  The  O  is  more  nearly  related  to  the 
tonic  C  than  A^\}  is.  On  the  other  hand,  Hauptmann  is  probably  right  in 
.f  considering  the  interval  D...E^\}  [=112  cents]  in  the  minor  mode,  as  one  in  which 
D  leads  to  E^\},  because  D  has  only  a  relationship  of  the  second  degree  to  the  tonic 
0,  although  its  relationship  is  certainly  closer  than  that  of  Bi  to  0. 

But  the  relation  of  D^\}  in  descending  passages  of  the  mode  of  the  minor 
Sixth  of  C  (the  old  Greek  Doric)  is  perfectly  similar  to  the  effect  of  ^1  in  the 
ascending  scale  of  C  major.  It  really  forms  a  kind  of  descending  leading  note, 
and  since  in  the  best  period  of  Greek  music  descending  passages  were  felt  as 
nobler  and  more  harmonious  than  ascending  ones,*  this  peculiarity  of  the  Dorio 
mode  may  have  been  of  special  importance  and  have  been  a  reason  for  the 
preference  given  to  this  scale.  The  cadence  with  the  chord  of  the  extreme  sharp 
Sixth  [ratio  128  :  225,  cents  976] — 

C    — JE;»[> +  (?...  c  ^ 

f  is  almosi  the  only  remnant  of  the  ancient  tonal  modes.  It  is  quite  isolated  and 
misunderstood.  This  is  a  (Greek)  Doric  cadence,  in  which  D^\}  and  Bi  are  both 
used  at  the  same  time  as  leading  notes  to  O.f 

The  relation  of  the  second  or  pa/rhypate  of  the  Greek  Doric  scale,  to  the  lowest 
tone  or  hypate,  seems  also  to  have  been  perfectly  well  felt  by  the  Greeks  them< 
selves,  to  judge  by  Ari8totle*s  remarks  in  the  3rd  and  4th  of  his  problems  on 
harmony.    I  cannot  abstain  from  adducing  them  here  because  they  admirably  and 

V  delicately  characterise  the  relation.  Aristotle  inquires  why  the  singer  feels  his 
voice  more  taxed  in  taking  the  parhypate  than  in  taking  the  hypatS,  although  they 
are  separated  by  so  small  an  interval.  The  hypate  is  sung,  he  says,  with  a  remission 
of  effort.  And  then  he  adds  that  in  order  to  reach  an  aim  easily  it  is  necessary  that 
in  addition  to  the  motive  which  determines  the  will,  the  kind  of  volitional  effort 
should  be  quite  feuniliar  and  easy  to  the  mind.t  The  effort  felt  in  singing  the  lead- 

*  Aristotle,  Problems  zix<  33.     [The  pas-  robots  9^  ir6vos'  xovovvra  84  fxaXXop  Sto^cfperoi* 

^  sage  has  already  been  cited  at  full,  p.  241(2',  Aid  rl  84  ravrvp  x'^^^h  I'V  84  iirw^v  pifBtmr 

note  I. — Translator,]  icdrot   ZUais   Ijcor^pas;  *H   8rt  fur*  kifiauos  1^ 

f  [This  cadence  is  a  union  of  the  ancient  ^^i},  koX  dfia  /xtrii.  r^v  tHtvraffuf  4ka^¥  rh 

Doric,  beginning  with  c,  rendered  harmonisable  ivot  fidXXttv  ;  Ath.  rainh  84  Ifoocc  irp6s  fiiay  ^^ccr- 

as  the  mode  of  the  minor  Sixth,  (c ... c2' b ... 0' b  Bcurii  itpbs  rairrfiv  ^  mpatr^imv  j^fuwa'  8€<  7^ 

. . . / . .  .pr . . .  a*  b  . . .  b'  b . . .  c',  p.  27&2,  note)  with  the  fi^rit  trwpoias  icol  Kwraffrda-€»s  ohc^undrris  r^ 

modem  minor,  beginning  with  c,  (c. .  .d. .  .«•  b . . ./  ^^i  irphs  riiv  fiofiKriffUf.    Arist.  ProbL  xix.  3,  4. 

...</...  a'  b  ...  bi .. .  c',)  and  will  be  more  partica-  [The  whole  passage  may  perhaps  be  translated 

larly  considered  in  the  next  chapter,  pp.  2o6d-  thus :  *  Why  do  those  who  sing  the  parhypaU 

308c.    The  intervals  expressed  in  cents  are  break  down  not  less  than  those  who  sing  the 

i)'b  386  F  204  O  386  B,  and  C  316  J7'b  386  G  nJete  and  higher  tones,  though  with  a  greater 

498  c— Translator.]  disagreement  (hiiartura)  ?    Is  it  because  they 

%  This  periphrasis  seems  to  me  to  render  sing  this  with  the  greatest  difficulty,  even  when 

correctly  the  last  clause  in  the  following  cita-  this  is  the  beginning  ?     Does  not  difficulty 

tion :   Ath  ri  r^v  trofnnrdrriv  $8orrcs  tAdhiora  arise  from  straining  [and  forcing]  the  voice  ? 

itiro^pfirYruvrtUy  oitx  i^Toy  ^  r^v  jrfirrip  KpX  rh.  6»t»^  This  occasions  effort,  and  things  done  with 

fitrii  84  9uurrdtrws  wKtiovos;  *H  8ri  xo^cin^crra  effort  are  most  apt  to  fail.    But  why  do  they 

rairjfy  fSovo'i,  icol  cStti  i|  kpxh ;  t^  84  xo^-c^o^  sing  the  parhypaU  with   difficulty,  and  yet 

9tk  riiv  iwh-wrw    [irat  wUcw]  riis  ^»tnis ;  iv  take  the  hypate  easily,  although  there  is  on^ 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


OTAP.xiv.  MEANING  OF  THE  LEADING  NOTE.  287 

ing  note  does  not  lie  in  the  larynx,  but  in  the  difficulty  we  feel  in  fixing  the  voice 
upon  it  by  mere  volition  while  another  tone  is  already  in  our  mind,  to  which  we 
desire  to  pass,  and  which  by  its  proximity  conducted  us  to  the  leading  note.  It  is 
not  till  we  reach  the  final  tone  that  we  feel  ourselves  at  home  and  at  rest,  and  this 
final  tone  is  sung  without  any  strain  on  the  will. 

Proximity  in  the  scale  then  gives  a  new  point  of  connection  between  two  tones, 
which  is  not  merely  active  in  the  case  of  the  leading  note,  just  considered,  but  also, 
as  already  mentioned,  in  interpolating  tones  between  two  others  in  the  chromatic 
and  enharmonic  modes.  Intervals  of  pitch  are  in  this  respect  analogous  to  mea- 
surements of  distance.  When  we  have  the  means  of  determining  one  point  (the 
tonic)  with  great  exactness  and  certainty,  we  are  able  by  its  means  to  determine 
other  points  with  certainty,  when  they  are  at  a  known  small  distance  from  it  (the 
interval  of  a  Semitone),  although  perhaps  we  could  not  have  determined  them  with 
so  much  certainty  independently.  Thus  the  astronomer  employs  his  fundamental  f 
fixed  stars,  of  which  the  positions  have  been  determined  with  the  greatest  possible 
accuracy,  for  accurately  determining  the  positions  of*  other  stars  in  their  neigh- 
bourhood. 

We  may  also  remark  that  the  interval  of  a  Semitone  plays  a  peculiar  part  as  the 
introduction  {appoggiatura)  to  another  note.  As  an  appoggiatura  in  a  melody  any 
tone  can  be  used,  even  when  not  in  its  scale,  provided  it  makes  the  interval  of  a 
Semitone  with  the  note  in  the  scale  which  it  introduces ;  but  a  foreign  tone  which 
makes  the  interval  of  a  whole  Tone  with  that  note  in  the  scale,  cannot  be  so  used. 
The  only  justification  of  this  use  of  the  Semitone  is  certainly  its  existence  as  a 
well-known  interval  in  the  diatonic  scale,  which  the  voice  can  sing  correctly  and 
the  ear  can  readily  appreciate,  even  when  the  relations  on  which  its  magnitude 
ddipends  are  not  clearly  sensible  in  the  passage  where  it  is  used.  Hence  also  no 
arbitrarily  chosen  small  interval  can  be  thus  employed.  Although  slight  changes 
in  the  interval  of  the  leading  note  may  be  introduced  by  practical  musician^Ho  give  ^ 
a  stronger  expression  to  its  tendency  towards  the  tonic,  they  must  never  go  so  fia.r 
as  to  make  those  changes  clearly  felt.* 

Hence  the  major  Seventh  in  its  character  of  leading  note  to  the  tonic  acquires 
a  new  and  closer  relationship  to  it,  unattainable  by  the  minor  Seventh.  And  in 
this  way  the  note  which  is  most  distantly  related  to  the  tonic  beeomes  peculiarly 
valuable  in  the  scale.  This  circumstance  has  continually  grown  in  importance  in 
modem  music,  which  aims  at  referring  every  tone  to  the  tonic  in  the  clearest  pos- 
sible manner ;  and  hence,  in  ascending  passages  going  to  the  tonic,  a  preference 
has  been  given  to  the  major  Seventh  in  all  modem  keys,  even  in  those  to  which  it  did 
not  properly  belong.  This  transformation  appears  to  have  begun  in  Europe  during 
the  period  of  polyphonic  music,  but  not  in  part  songs  only,  for  we  find  it  also  in  the 
bomophonio  Gantus  firrmiB  of  the  Eoman  Catholic  Church.  It  was  blamed  in  an 
edict  of  Pope  John  XXII.,  in  1322,  and  in  consequence  the  sharpening  of  the  lead- 
ing note  was  omitted  in  writing,  but  was  supplied  by  the  singers,  a  practice  which  ^ 
Winterfeld  believes  to  have  been  followed  by  Protestant  musical  composers  even 
down  to  the  sixteenth  and  seventeenth  centuries,  because  it  had  once  come  into 
use.  And  this  makes  it  impossible  to  determine  exactly  what  were  the  steps  by 
which  this  change  in  the  old  tonal  modes  was  effected.f 

Even  to  the  present  day,  according  to  A.  v.  Oettingen's  report,^  the  Esthonians 

a  diesis  (Semitone  or  Qaartertone)  between  clearly  a  connection  in  the  writer's  mind  be- 

them  ?    Is  it  because  the  hypatS  is  snng  with  tween    Sicbroo-is,   HffrturiSy    and    Kardaraffis^ 

a  remission  of  eiffort,  and  at  the  same  time  it  which  influenced  his  reasoning,  but  evaporates 

is   easy  to  go  upwards   after  getting  oneself  in  translation. — Translator.] 

together  for  the  effort  {avcrouris)  ?    For  the  ♦   [See  App.  XX.  sect.  G.  art.'  6.—TranS' 

same  reason  it  is  easy  to  sing  what  leads  up  latorJ] 

to  any  note,  or  the  paraneU.    For  the  will  f  Def  evangeUsche  Kirchengesang,    Leip- 

requires  not  only  conscious  thought  {Hwota)  zig,  1843,  vol.  i.  introduction. 

but  an  inclination  {Kardtrrcuris)  which  is  per-  J  Das    Harmoniesystem   in   dualer  Ent- 

fectly  familiar  to  the  habit  of  mind  {^Bos).*  wickelung,    Dorpat  und  Leipzig,  1866,  p.  113, 

The  passage  is  very  difficult,  and  there  was 

Digitized  by  VjOOQlC 


288  MEANING  OP  THE  LEADING  NOTE.  part  m. 

struggle  against  singing  the  leading  note  in  minor  scales,  although  it  may  be  clearly 
struck  on  the  organ. 

Among  the  ancient  tonal  modes,  the  Greek  Lydian  (major  mode)  and  the  un- 
melodic  Hypolydian  (mode  of  the  Fifth,  p.  269a,  No.  7)  had  the  major  Seventh  as 
the  leading  note  to  the  tonic,  and  hence  the  first  was  developed  into  the  principal 
tonal  mode  of  modem  music,  the  major  mode.  The  Greek  Ionic  (mode  of  the 
Fourth)  differed  from  it  only  in  having  a  minor  Seventh.  On  simply  altering  this 
into  a  major  Seventh,  this  mode  also  became  major.  On  giving  a  major  Seventh 
to  each  of  the  other  three,  they  gradually  converged  to  our  present  minor  mode 
during  the  seventeenth  century.  From  the  Greek  Phrygian  (mode  of  the  minor 
Seventh)  by  changing  B\}  into  By  we  obtain 

THE   ASCENDING  MINOR   SCALE. 

'  G„.D...E'\}...F.„G...Ai..,By...c 

as  we  had  already  found  from  a  simple  consideration  of  the  relationship  of  tones 
[p.  2746,  No.  2].  The  Greek  Hypodoric  or  EoKc  (mode  of  the  minor  Third),  which 
answers  to  our  descending  minor  scale,  gives  on  changing  B^\}  into  ^1, 

THE  INSTRUMENTAL  MINOR  SCALE. 

C ...  D ...  E^\}  ...  F ...  G  ...  A'\}  ...  5, ...  c 

which  is  difficult  for  singers  to  execute,  on  account  of  the  interval  A^\}  ...  5, 
[=  ratio  64  :  75,  cents  274J,  but  frequently  occurs  in  modem  music  both  ascending 
and  descending. 

The  Greek  Doric  (mode  of  the  minor  Sixth)  with  a  major  instead  of  a  minor 
Seventh,  is  still  discoverable  in  the  final  cadence  mentioned  on  p.  286^. 

The  general  introduction  of  the  leading  tone  represents,  therefore,  a  continually 
increasing  consistency  in  the  development  of  a  feeling  for  the  predominance  of  the 
«r  tonic  in  a  scale.  By  this  change,  not  only  is  the  variety  of  character  in  the  ancient 
tonal  modes  seriously  injured,  and  the  wealth  of  previous  means  of  expression 
essentially  diminished,  but  even  the  links  of  the  chain  of  tones  in  the  scale  were 
disrupted  or  disturbed.  We  have  seen  that  the  most  ancient  theory  made  tonal 
systems  consist  of  series  of  Fifths,  and  that  each  system  had  at  first  four  and  after- 
wards six  intervals  of  a  Fifth.  The  predominance  of  a  tonic  as  the  single  focus 
of  the  system  was  not  yet  indicated,  at  least  externally ;  it  became  apparent  at 
most  by  a  limitation  of  the  number  of  Fifths  to  contain  those  tones  only  which 
occurred  in  the  natural  scale.  All  Greek  tonal  modes  may  be  formed  from  the 
tones  in  the  series  of  Fifths — 

F±C±G±D±A±E±B. 

Directly  we  proceed  to  the  natural  intonation  of  Thirds,  the  series  of  Fifths  ia 
interrupted  by  an  imperfect  Fifth,  as  in 

m  F  ±  C  ±  G  ±  D  ...  Ay  ±  El  ±  Bi 

where  the  Fifth  D...A1  [=  680  cents]  is  imperfect.  And  when  finally  the  sharp 
leading  note  is  introduced,  as  by  the  use  of  G^if^  for  G  in  Ay  minor,  the  series 
is  entirely  interrupted  [0 :  (?2  J  =  16  :  25  =  772  cents]. 

In  the  gradual  development  of  the  diatonic  system,  therefore,  the  various  links 
of  the  chain  which  bound  the  tones  together  were  sacrificed  successively  to  the 
desire  of  connecting  all  the  tones  in  a  scale  with  one  central  tone,  the  tonic.  And 
in  exact  proportion  to  the  degree  with  which  this  was  carried  out,  the  conception  of 
tonality  consciously  developed  itself  in  the  minds  of  musicians. 

The  further  development  of  the  European  tonal  system  is  due  to  the  cultivation 
of  harmony,  which  will  occupy  us  in  the  next  chapter. 

But  before  leaving  our  present  subject,  some  doubtful  points  have  still  to  be 
considered.  In  the  preceding  chapter  I  have  shewn  that  the  melodic  relationship 
of  tones  can  be  made  to  depend  upon  their  upper  partials,  precisely  in  the  same 

Digitized  by  VjOOQlC 


CHAP.  XIV.  MELODY  IN   SIMPLE   TONES.  289 

way  as  their  consonance  was  shewn  to  be  determined  in  Chapter  X.  Now  this 
method  of  explanation  may  in  a  certain  sense  be  considered  to  agree  with  the 
favourite  assertion  that  *  melody  is  resolved  harmony,*  on  which  mtisicians  do 
not  hesitate  to  form  musical  systems  without  staying  to  inquire  how  harmonies 
could  have  been  resolved  into  melodies  at  times  and  places  where  harmonies  had 
either  never  been  heard,  or  were,  after  hearing,  repudiated.  According  to  our 
explanation,  at  least,  the  same  physical  peculiarities  in  the  composition  of  musical 
tones,  which  determined  consonances  for  tones  struck  simultaneously,  would  also 
determine  melodic  relations  for  tones  struck  in  succession.  The  former  then  would 
not  be  the  reason  for  the  latter,  as  the  above  phrase  suggests,  but  both  would  have 
a  common  cause  in  the  natural  formation  of  musical  tones. 

Again,  in  consonance  we  found  other  peculiar  relations,  due  to  combinational 
tones,  which  become  effective  even  when  simple  tones,  or  tones  with  few  and  faint 
upper  partials,  are  struck  simultaneously.  I  have  already  shewn  that  combina-  ^ 
tional  tones  very  imperfectly  replace  the  effect  of  upper  partial  tones  in  a  con- 
sonance, and  that  consequently  a  chord  formed  of  simple  tones  is  wanting  in 
brightness  and  character,  the  distinctions  between  consonance  and  dissonance 
being  only  very  imperfectly  developed. 

In  melodic  passages,  however,  combinational  tones  do  not  occur,  and  hence  the 
question  arises  as  to  how  far  a  melodic  effect  could  be  produced  by  a  succession  of 
simple  tones.  There  is  no  doubt  that  we  can  recognise  melodies  which  we  have 
already  heard,  when  they  are  executed  on  the  stopped  pipes  of  an  organ,  or  are 
whistled  with  the  mouth,  or  merely  struck  on  a  glass  or  wood  or  steel  harmonicon, 
as  a  musical  box,  or  are  played  on  bells.  But  there  is  also  no  doubt  that  all  these 
instruments,  which  generate  simple  tones,  either  alone  or  accompanied  by  weak 
and  remote  inharmonic  secondary  tones,  are  incapable  of  producing  any  effective 
melodic  impression  without  an  accompaniment  of  musical  instruments  proper. 
They  may  be  often  extremely  effective  for  performing  single  parts  when  accom-  ^ 
panied  by  the  organ,  or  the  orchestra,  or  a  pianoforte,  but  by  themselves  they  produce 
very  poor  music  indeed,  which  degenerates  into  absolute  unpleasantness  when  the 
inharmonic  secondary  tones  are  somewhat  too  loud. 

We  are  bound,  however,  to  give  some  reason  why  any  impression  of  melody  at 
all  can  be  produced  by  such  instnmients. 

Now  we  must  first  remember  that,  as  shewn  at  the  end  of  Chapter  VII.,  the 
actual  construction  of  the  ear  favours  the  generation  of  weak  harmonic  upper  par- 
tials within  the  ear  itself,  when  powerful  but  objectively  simple  tones  are  sounded. 
Hence  it  is  at  most  very  weak  objectively  simple  tones  which  can  be  regarded  as 
also  subjectively  simple. 

Next,  there  is  an  effect  of  memory  to  be  brought  into  account.  Supposing 
that  I  have  been  used  to  hear  Fifths  taken  at  all  possible  pitches,  and  have  recog- 
nised them  by  aural  sensation  as  having  a  very  close  melodic  relationship,  I  should 
know  the  magnitude  of  this  interval  by  experience  for  every  tone  in  the  scale,  ^ 
and  should  retain  the  knowledge  thus  acquired  by  the  action  of  a  man's  memory 
of  sensations,  even  of  those  for  which  he  has  no  verbal  expression. 

When,  then,  I  hear  such  an  interval  executed  on  tuning-forks,  I  am  able  to 
recognise  it  as  an  interval  I  have  often  heard,  although  its  tones  have  either  none, 
or  only  some  faint  remnants  of  those  upper  partials  which  formerly  gave  it  a  right 
to  be  considered  as  a  favourite  interval  of  close  melodic  relationship.  And  just  in 
the  same  way  I  shall  be  able  to  recognise,  as  previously  known,  other  melodic  pas- 
sages or  whole  melodies  which  are  executed  in  simple  tones,  and  even  if  I  hear  a 
melody  for  the  first  time  in  this  way,  whistled  with  the  mouth  or  chimed  by  a  clock, 
or  struck  on  a  glass  harmonicon,  I  should  be  able  to  complete  it  by  imagining  how 
it  would  soimd  if  executed  on  a  real  musical  instrument,  as  the  voice  or  a  violin. 

A  practised  musician  is  able  to  form  a  conception  of  a  melody  by  merely  reading 
the  notes.  If  we  give  the  prime  tones  of  these  notes  on  a  glass  harmonicon,  we  give 
a  firmer  basis  to  the  conception  by  really  exciting  a  large  portion  of  the  impression 

Digitized  by  V^ODQIC 


290  MELODY  IN  SIMPLE   TONES.  pabt  iu. 

on  the  senses  which  the  melody  would  have  produced  if  sung.  Simple  tones,  how- 
ever, merely  exhibit  an  outline  of  the  melody.  All  that  gives  the  melody  its  charm 
is  absent.  We  know,  indeed,  the  individual  intervals  which  it  contains,  but  we  have 
no  inamediate  impression  on  our  senses  which  serves  to  distinguish  those  which  are 
distantly  from  those  which  are  closely  related,  or  the  related  from  the  totally  un- 
related. Observe  the  great  difference  between  merely  whistling  a  melody  or  play- 
ing it  on  a  violin  ;  between  striking  it  on  a  glass  harmonicon  or  on  the  piano  I  The 
difference  is  somewhat  of  the  same  kind  as  that  between  viewing  a  single  photograph 
of  a  landscape,  and  seeing  two  corresponding  photographs  of  it  through  a  stereo- 
scope. The  first  enables  me,  by  means  of  my  memory,  to  form  a  conception  of  the 
relative  distances  of  its  parts,  and  this  conception  may  be  often  very  satisfactory. 
But  the  stereoscopic  fusion  of  the  two  figures  gives  me  the  real  impression  on  the 
senses  which  the  relative  distances  of  the  parts  of  the  landscape  would  have  them- 
[  selves  produced,  and  which  I  am  obhged  in  the  case  of  a  single  image  to  supply  by 
experience  and  memory.  Hence  the  stereoscopic  picture  is  more  lively  than  the 
simple  perspective  view,  exactly  in  the  same  way  as  immediate  impressions  on  our 
senses  are  more  Hvely  than  our  recollections. 

The  case  seems  to  be  the  same  for  melodies  executed  in  simple  tones.  We 
recognise  the  melodies  when  we  have  heard  them  otherwise  performed ;  we  can 
even,  if  we  have  sufi&cient  musical  imagination,  picture  to  ourselves  how  they 
would  sound  if  executed  by  other  instruments,  but  they  are  decidedly  without  the 
immediate  impression  on  the  senses  which  gives  music  its  charm. 


CHAPTER  XV. 

THE   CONSONANT   CHORDS  OF  THE   TONAL   MODES. 

^  Polyphony  was  the  form  in  which  music  for  several  voices  first  attained  a  certain 
degree  of  artistic  perfection.  The  peculiar  characteristic  of  this  style  of  music 
was  that  several  voices  were  singing  each  its  own  independent  melody  at  the  same 
time,  which  might  be  a  repetition  of  the  melodies  already  sung  by  the  other  voices, 
or  else  quite  a  different  one.  Under  these  circumstances  each  voice  had  to  obey 
the  general  law  of  tonaHty  common  to  the  construction  of  all  melodies,  and,  more- 
over, every  tone  of  a  polyphonic  passage  had  to  be  referred  to  the  same  tonic. 
Hence  each  voice  had  to  commence  separately  on  the  tonic  or  some  tone  closely 
related  to  it,  and  to  close  in  the  same  way.  In  practice  each  part  of  a  polyphonic 
piece  was  made  to  begin  with  the  tonic  or  its  Octave.  This  fulfilled  the  law  of 
tonality,  but  necessitated  the  closing  of  a  polyphonic  piece  with  a  unison. 

The  reason  why  higher  Octaves  might  accompany  the  tonic  at  the  close,  lies,  as 
we  saw  in  the  last  chapter,  in  the  fa^i  that  higher  Octaves  are  merely  repetitions  of 
portions  of  the  fundamental  tone.  Hence  by  adding  its  Octave  to  the  tonic  at  the 
H  close,  we  merely  reinforce  part  of  its  compound  tone  ;  no  new  compound  or  simple 
tone  is  added,  and  the  union  of  all  the  tones  contains  only  the  constituents  of  the 
tonic  itself. 

The  same  is  true  for  all  the  other  partial  tones  which  are  contained  in  the  tonic. 
The  next  step  in  the  development  of  the  final  chord  was  to  add  the  Twelfth  of  the 
tonic.  Now  the  chord  c...c'±^'  contains  no  element  which  is  not  also  a  con- 
stituent of  the  compound  tone  c  when  sounded  alone,  and  consequently,  being  a 
mere  representative  of  the  single  musical  tone  o,  it  is  suitable  for  the  termination 
of  a  piece  of  music  having  the  tonic  c. 

Nay  even  the  chord  &±g\..c"  might  be  so  used,  for  when  it  is  struck  we  hear, 
weakly  indeed,  but  still  sensibly,  the  combinational  tone  c,  so  that  the  whole  mass 
of  tone  again  contains  nothing  more  than  the  constituents  of  the  tone  c.  It  must 
be  owned,  however,  that  this  combination  would  answer  to  a  rather  unusual  quality 
of  tone,  with  a  proportionably  weak  prime  partial. 

On  the  other  hand,  it  was  not  possible  to  use  the  chords  c.c'.../'  or  c'.../'dic" 

Digitized  by  VjOOQlC 


CHAP.   XV. 


MEANING   OF  THE   TONIC  CHORD. 


291 


to  end  a  piece  having  the  tonic  c,  although  these  chords  are  consonances  as  well  as 
the  preceding,  because  /  is  not  an  element  of  tlie  compound  tone  c,  and  hence  the 
closing  chord  would  contain  something  which  was  not  the  tonic  at  all.  It  is  here 
probably  that  we  have  to  look  for  the  reason  why  some  medieval  theoreticians 
wished  to  reckon  the  Fourth  among  the  dissonances.  But  perfect  consonance  was 
not  sufficient  to  make  an  interval  available  for  the  final  chord.  There  was  a  second 
condition  which  the  theoreticians  did  not  clearly  understand.  The  tones  of  the 
final  chord  had  to  be  constituents  of  the  compound  tone  of  the  tonic.  This  was 
the  only  case  in  which  those  tones  could  be  employed. 

The  Sixth  of  the  tonic  is  as  ill  suited  as  the  Fourth  for  use  in  the  final  chord. 
But  the  major  Third  can  be  used,  because  it  occurs  as  the  fifth  partial  tone  of  the 
tonic.  Since  the  qualities  of  tone  which  are  fit  for  music  generally  allow  the  fifth 
and  sixth  partial  tone  to  be  audible,  but  make  the  higher  partials  either  entirely 
inaudible  or  at  least  very  funt,  and  since,  moreover,  the  seventh  partial  is  dissonant  ^ 
i¥ith  the  fifth,  sixth,  and  eighth,  and  is  not  used  in  the  scale,  the  series  of  tones 
available  for  the  closing  chord  terminates  with  the  Third.  Thus  we  actually  find 
down  to  the  beginning  of  the  eighteenth  century,  that  the  final  chord  has  either 
no  Third,  or  only  a  major  Third,  even  in  tonal  modes  which  contain  only  the  minor 
and  not  the  major  Third  of  the  tonic.  To  attain  fulness,  it  was  preferred  to  do 
violence  to  the  scale  by  using  the  major  Third  in  the  closing  chord.  The  minor 
Third  of  the  tonic  can  never  stand  for  a  constituent  of  its  compound  tone.  Hence 
it  was  originally  as  much  forbidden  as  the  Fourth  and  Sixth  of  the  tonic.  Be- 
fore a  minor  chord  could  be  used  to  close  a  piece  of  music  the  feeling  for  harmony 
bad  to  be  cultivated  in  a  new  direction. 

The  ear  is  the  more  satisfied  with  a  closing  major  chord,  the  more  closely  the 
order  of  the  tones  used  imitates  the  arrangement  of  the  partial  tones  in  a  compound. 
8ince  in  modem  music  the  upper  voice  is  most  conspicuous,  and  hence  has  the 
principal  melody,  this  voice  must  usually  finish  with  the  tonic.  Bearing  this  in  f 
mind,  we  can  use  any  of  the  following  chords  for  the  close  (combinational  tones 
are  added  as  crotchets) : — 

12345 


5 


rz2i 


=g= 


IZ2I 


izar 


122= 


m 


IC2I 


r?2= 


r 


r 


In  the  chords  i  and  2  all  the  notes  coincide  with  partials  of  0,  and  they 
therefore  most  closely  resemble  the  compound  tone  C  itself.  And  then  closer 
positions  of  the  chord  can  be  substituted,  provided  they  resemble  the  first  by  ^ 
having  C  for  the  fundamental  tone  as  in  3,  4,  5.  They  still  retain  sufficient  resem- 
blance to  the  compound  tone  of  the  low  G  to  be  used  in  its  place.  Moreover,  the 
combinational  tones,  written  as  crotchets  in  3,  4  and  5,  assist  in  the  effect  of 
making  the  deeper  partials  of  the  compound  C,  at  least  faintly,  audible.  But  the 
first  two  positions  always  give  the  most  satisfactory  close.  The  tendency  towards 
a  deep  final  tone  in  harmonic  music  is  very  characteristic,  and  I  beheve  that  the 
above  is  its  proper  explanation.  There  is  nothing  of  the  kind  in  the  construction 
of  homophonic  melodies.    It  is  peculiar  to  the  bass  of  part  music. 

Precisely  in  the  same  way  that  the  tonic,  when  used  as  the  bass  of  its  major 
chord  at  the  close,  gives  it  a  resemblance  to  its  own  compound  tone,  and  is  hence 
felt  as  the  essential  tone  of  the  chord,  all  major  chords  sound  best  when  the 
lowest  tone  of  their  closest  triad  position  (No.  4,  p.  219c)  is  made  the  bass.  The 
other  major  chords  in  the  scale  are  those  on  its  Fourth  and  Fifth,  and  hence  for 
the  scale  of  C  major,  are  F+Ai  —  C  and  G+^i— i>.    Hence  if  we  make  the 

Digitized  by  V^JiQQQlC 


292        REPLACEMENT  OF  COMPOUND  TONES  BY  CHORDS,    pabt  m. 

harmony  of  a  piece  of  masic  to  consist  of  these  major  chords  only,  each  having 
its  fundamental  tone  in  the  bass,  the  effect  is  almost  that  of  a  compound  tonic  in 
different  qualities  of  tone  passing  into  its  two  nearest  related  compound  tones,  the 
Fourth  and  Fifth.  This  makes  the  harmonisation  transparent  and  definite,  but  it 
would  be  too  uniform  for  long  pieces.  Modem  popular  tunes,  songs  and  dances, 
are  however,  as  is  well  known,  constructed  in  this  manner.  The  people,  and 
generally  persons  of  small  musical  cultivation,  can  be  pleased  only  by  extremely 
simple  and  intelligible  musical  relations.  Now  the  relations  of  the  tones  are 
generally  much  easier  to  feel  with  distinctness  in  harmonised  than  in  homophonic 
music.  In  the  latter  the  feeling  of  relationship  of  tone  depends  solely  on  the 
sameness  of  pitch  of  two  partials  in  two  Consecutive  musical  tones.  But  when  we 
hear  the  second  compound  tone  we  can  at  most  remember  the  first,  and  hence  we 
are  driven  to  complete  the  comparison  by  an  act  of  memory.  The  consonance,  on 
^  the  other  hand,  gives  the  relation  by  an  immediate  act  of  sensation ;  we  are  no 
longer  driven  to  have  recourse  to  memory ;  we  hear  beats,  or  there  is  a  roughness 
in  the  combined  sound,  when  the  proper  relations  are  not  preserved.  Again,  when 
two  chords  having  a  common  note  occur  in  succession,  our  recognition  of  their 
relationship  does  not  depend  upon  weak  upper  partials,  but  upon  the  comparison  of 
two  independent  notes,  having  the  same  force  as  the  other  notes  of  the  corre- 
sponding chord. 

When,  for  example,  I  ascend  from  C  to  its  Sixth  Ai,l  recognise  their  mutual 
relationship  in  an  unaccompanied  melody,  by  the  fact  that  «',,  the  fifth  partial  of 
C,  which  is  already  very  weak,  is  identical  with  the  third  partial  of  -4i.  But  if  I 
accompany  the  -4,  with  the  chord  jP+^Ii  —  c,  I  hear  the  former  c  sound  on  power- 
fully in  the  chord,  and  know  by  immediate  sensation  that  Ai  and  C  are  consonant, 
and  that  both  of  them  are  constituents  of  the  compound  tone  F, 

When  I  pass  melodically  from  C  to  i9i  or  X),  I  am  obliged  to  imagine  a  kind  of 
%  mute  G  between  them,  in  order  to  recognise  their  relationship,  which  is  of  the 
second  degree.    But  if  I  audibly  sustain  the  note  G  while  the  others  are  sounded, 
their  common  relationship  becomes  really  sensible  to  my  ear. 

Habituation  to  the  tonal  relations  so  evidently  displayed  in  harmonic  music, 
has  had  an  indisputable  influence  on  modem  musical  taste.  Unaccompanied 
songs  no  longer  please  us ;  they  seem  poor  and  incomplete.  But  if  merely  the 
twanging  of  a  guitar  adds  the  fundamental  chords  of  the  key,  and  indicates  the 
harmonic  relations  of  the  tones,  we  are  satisfied.  Again,  we  cannot  fail  to  see 
that  the  clearer  perception  of  tonal  relationship  in  harmonic  music  has  greatly 
increased  the  practicable  variety  in  the  relations  of  tones,  by  allowing  those  which 
are  less  marked  to  be  freely  used,  and  has  also  rendered  possible  the  construction 
of  long  musical  pieces  which  require  powerful  links  to  connect  their  parts  into  one 
whole. 

The  closest  and  simplest  relation  of  the  tones  is  reached  in  the  major  mode, 

^  when  all  the  tones  of  a  melody  are  treated  as  constituents  of  the  compound  tone 

of  the  tonic,  or  of  the  Fifth  above  or  the  Fifth  below  it.     By  this  means  all  the 

relations  of  tones  are  reduced  to  the  simplest  and  closest  relation  existing  in 

any  musical  system — that  of  the  Fifth. 

The  relation  of  the  chord  of  the  dominant  G  to  that  of  the  tonic  C,  is  some- 
what different  from  that  of  the  chord  of  the  subdominant  F  to  the  tonic  chord. 
When  we  pass  from  C-^-Ei—G  to  G-^Bi—d  we  use  a  compound  tone,  G,  which 
is  already  contained  in  the  first  chord,  and  is  consequently  properly  prepared,  while 
at  the  same  time  such  a  step  leads  us  to  those  degrees  of  the  scale  which  are  most 
distant  firom  the  tonic,  and  have  only  an  indirect  relationship  with  it.  Hence  this 
passage  forms  a  distinct  progression  in  the  harmony,  which  is  at  once  well  assured 
and  properly  based.  It  is  quite  different  with  the  passage  from  C+J5J|— G  to 
F-^Ai^c,  The  compound  tone  F  is  not  prepared  in  the  first  chord,  and  it  has 
therefore  to  be  discovered  and  struck.  Hence  the  justification  of  this  passage  as 
correct  and  closely  related,  is  not  complete  until  the  step  is  actually  made  and  it  is 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


CHAP.  XV, 


HARMONY  OF  THE  MAJOR  MODE. 


293 


felt  that  tlie  chord  of  F  contains  no  tones  which  are  not  directly  related  to  the  tonic 
(7.  In  the  passage  from  the  chord  of  0  to  that  of  F,  therefore,  we  miss  that  distinct 
and  well-assured  progression  which  marked  the  passage  from  the  chord  of  C  to  that 
of  G,  Bat  as  a  compensation,  the  progression  from  the  chord  of  C  to  that  of  F 
bas  a  softer  and  calmer  kind  of  heaaty,  due,  perhaps,  to  its  keeping  within  tones 
directly  related  to  the  tonic  C,  Popular  music,  however,  favours  the  other  passage 
from  the  tonic  to  the  Fifth  ahove  (hence  called  the  dominant  of  the  key),  and  many 
of  the  simpler  popular  songs  and  dances  consist  merely  of  an  interchange  of  tonic 
and  dominant  chords.  Hence  also  the  common  harmonicon  (accordion,  German 
concertina),  which  is  arranged  for  them,  gives  the  tonic  chord  on  opening  the 
bellows,  and  the  dominant  chord  on  closing  them.  The  Fifth  below  the  tonic  is 
called  the  sz^idominant  of  the  key.  Its  chord  is  seldom  introduced  at  all  into  usual 
popular  melodies,  except,  perhaps,  once  near  the  close,  to  restore  the  equilibrium 
of  the  harmony,  which  had  chiefly  inclined  towards  the  dominant. 

When  a  section  of  a  piece  of  music  terminates  with  the  passage  of  the  dominant 
into  the  tonic  chord,  musicians  call  the  close  a  complete  cadence.  We  thus  return  from 
tbe  tones  most  distantly  related  to  the  tonic,  to  the  tonic  itself,  and,  as  befits  a  close, 
make  a  distinct  passage  from  the  remotest  parts  of  the  scale  to  the  centre  of  the 
system  itself.  If,  on  the  other  hand,  we  close  by  passing  from  the  subdominant  to  the 
tonic  chord,  the  result  is  called  an  imperfect  or  plagal  cadence.  The  tones  of  the 
subdominant  triad  are  all  directly  related  to  the  tonic,  so  that  we  are  already  close 
upon  the  tonic  before  we  pass  over  to  it.  Hence  the  imperfect  cadence  corresponds 
to  a  much  quieter  return  of  the  music  to  tbe  tonic  chord,  and  the  progression  is 
much  less  distinct  than  before. 

In  tbe  complete  cadence  tbe  chord  of  tbe  tonic  follows  that  of  the  dominant, 
but  to  preserve  the  equilibrium  of  the  system  in  relation  to  tbe  subdominant,  its 
chord  is  made  to  precede  that  of  the  dominant  as  in  i  or  2. 


IF 


IS 


-4- 


^ 


-<sf- 


"Ts — : 


ESE 


321 


-^     r:>- 


IZ2I 


A^^ 


"7S>" 


This  succession  really  forms  tbe  complete  close,  by  bringing  all  the  tones  of  the 
whole  scale  together  again,  and  thus  in  conclusion  collecting  and  fixing  every  part 
of  tbe  key. 

Tbe  major  mode,  as  we  bavei  seen,  permits  tbe  requisitions  of  tonality  to  be 
most  easily  and  completely  united  with  harmonic  completeness.  Every  tone  of  its 
scale  can  be  employed  as  a  constituent  of  tbe  musical  tone  of  tbe  tonic,  the  domi  IT 
nant,  or  the  subdominant,  because  these  fundamental  tones  of  tbe  mode  are  also 
fundamental  tones  of  major  chords.  This  is  not  equally  tbe  case  in  tbe  other 
ancient  tonal  modes. 


I.  Majob  mode  * 


Mode  of  the 

FOUBTH  * 


major 

^"~  •  *       "<k 
f  +  ai   — c   +«!    "  g   +&1— ^ 

• , ^ . 

major  major 

major 

/  +  ai    -  c   +  ei    -  g  "  b^\}  +  d 

major  minor 


*  [Of  course  when  the  modes  are  thas 
reduced  to  harmonic  combinations,  the  tones 
ol  the  old  modes,  as  given  in  footnote  to  p.  268c, 


are  all  altered,  and  become  those  in  footnote 
to  p.  274c.  See  also  App.  XX.  sect.  £,  arts.  9 
and  10,— Translator.] 


Digitized  by  V^OOQIC 


294 


AMBIGUITY  OF   THE   MINOR  CHORD. 


PART  m. 


3.  Mode  op  the 
MiNOB  Seventh  * 


4.  Mode  op  the 

MINOB  Thibd* 
{minor  mode) 

5.  Mode  op  the 

MINOB  Sixth* 


minor 

/   4-  ai    -  c    -  e^\}-h  g    -  b^\}+  d 

major  minor 

minor 

/  -  a^\>+  c   -  e^\>+  g  -  2>'t>+  ^ 


minor 


minor 


minor 

5t?-  ^*t>+  /  -  a»t?+  c   -  e^\}+  g 

minor  minor 


In  the  minor  chords,  the  Third  does  not  belong  to  the  compomid  tone  of  its  fdnda- 
%  mental  note,  and  hence  cannot  appear  as  a  constituent  of  its  quality ;  so  that  the 
relation  of  all  the  parts  of  a  minor  chord  to  the  fundamental  note  is  not  ao  im- 
mediate as  that  for  the  major  chord,  and  this  is  a  source  of  difficulty  in  the  final 
chord.  For  this  reason  we  find  almost  all  popular  dance  and  song  music  written 
m  the  major  mode  f ;  indeed,  the  minor  mode  forms  a  rare  exception.  The  people 
must  have  the  clearest  and  simplest  intelligibility  in  their  music,  and  this  can  only 
be  furnished  by  the  major  mode.  But  there  was  nothing  like  this  predominance  of 
the  major  key  in  homophonic  music.  For  the  same  reason  the  harmonic  accom- 
paniment of  chorales  in  major  keys  was  developed  with  tolerable  completeness  as 
early  as  the  sixteenth  century,  so  that  many  of  them  correspond  with  the  cultivated 
musical  taste  of  the  present  day;  but  the  harmonic  treatment  of  the  minor  and 
the  other  ecclesiastical  modes  was  still  in  a  very  unsettled  condition,  and  strikes 
modem  ears  as  very  strange. 

In  a  major  chord  c+^i  —g,  we  may  regard  both  g  and  e,  as  constituents  of  the 
f  compound  tone  of  c,  but  neither  c  nor  g  as  constituents  of  the  compound  tone  of  e^, 
and  neither  c  nor  Ci  as  constituents  of  the  compound  tone  of  g.t  .  Hence  the  major 
chord  c+Ci^g  is  completely  unambiguous,  and  can  be  compared  only  with  the 
compound  tone  of  c,  and  consequently  c  is  the  predominant  tone  in  the  chord,  its 
rooty  or,  in  Rameau's  language,  its  fundamental  bass  ;  and  neither  of  the  other  two 
tones  in  the  chord  has  the  sHghtest  claim  to  be  so  considered. 

In  the  minor  chord  c—e^\}+gj  the  ^  is  a  constituent  of  the  compound  tones  of 
both  c  and  e^\}.  Neither  e^^  nor  c  occurs  in  either  of  the  other  two  compound 
tones  c,  g.  Hence  it  is  clear  that  g  at  least  is  a  dependent  tone.  But,  on  the  other 
hand,  this  minor  chord  can  be  regarded  either  as  a  compound  tone  of  c  with  an 
added  e^\}  or  as  a  compound  tone  of  e^\}  with  an  added  c.  Both  views  are  enter- 
tained at  different  times,  but  the  first  usually  prevails.  If  we  regard  the  chord  as 
the  compound  tone  of  c,  we  find  g  for  its  third  partial,  while  the  foreign  tone  6^t> 
only  occupies  the  place  of  the  weak  fifth  partial  e|.  But  if  we  regarded  the  chord 
f  as  a  compound  tone  of  e^\},  although  the  weak  fifth  partial  g  would  be  properly  repre- 
sented, the  stronger  third  partial,  which  ought  to  be  b^\},  is  replaced  by  the  foreign 
tone  c.  Hence  in  modem  music  we  usually  find  the  minor  chord  c—e*b+^  treated 
as  if  its  root  or  fundamental  bass  were  c,  so  that  the  chord  appears  as  a  some- 
what altered  and  obscured  compound  tone  of  c.  But  the  chord  also  occurs  in  the 
position  e^\}-^g.,,c  (or  better  still  as  e^\}-^g...c^)  even  in  the  key  of  B^\}  major,  as  a 
substitute  for  the  chord  of  the  subdominant  e'j^.  Rameau  then  calls  it  the  chord  of 
the  great  Sixth  [in  Enghsh  '  added  Sixth  '],  and,  more  correctly  than  most  modem 
theoreticians,  regards  e^\}  as  its  fundamental  bass.§ 

♦  [See  p.  293,  note.]      t  [T^is  remark  does  not  apply  to  old  English  moBio,— Translator.^ 


X  [Taking  only  six  partials,  we  have  for — 


Oompoimd  ToneB 

Simple  Partial  Tones 

12345      6 

C 

C     c     g     &    e\     g' 
Ex   e,    6,    e\   g'^l  b\ 

^1 

G, 

0    g     d'    g'    b\    d" 

£'b 

£"bc'b6'bc"b<7'    6"b. 

— Translator. 1 

§  [The  scale  of  B^  b  major  has  the  chords 
«^b  +  gr— 6'b  +  d-/*  +  a— c*;  hence,  regarding 
the  chord  as  made  up  of  the  notes  of  this 
scale,  it  would  be  c^  \  e'b  +  (7,  which  is  not  a 
minor  chord  at  all,  like  c-e'b  +  f7,  because  it 
has  a  Pythagorean  in  place  of  a  just  minor 
Third.  It  was  only  tempered  intonation  which 
confused  the  iwc?;c^|e^^|^f£^  wUl   be 


CHAP.  XV,  AMBIGUITY   OF  THE  MINOR  CHORD.  295 

When  it  is  important  to  guide  the  ear  in  selecting  one  or  other  of  these  two 
meanings  of  the  minor  chord,  the  root  intended  may  be  emphasised  by  giving  it  a 
low  position  or  by  throwing  several  voices  upon  it.  The  low  position  of  the  root 
allows  such  other  tones  as  could  be  fitted  into  its  compound  tone,  to  be  considered 
directly  as  its  partials,  whereas  the  low  compound  tone  itself  cannot  be  considered 
as  the  partial  of  another  much  higher  tone.  In  the  first  half  of  last  century,  when 
the  minor  chord  was  first  used  as  a  close,  composers  endeavoured  to  give  pro- 
minence to  the  tonic  by  increasing  the  loudness  of  the  tonic  note  in  comparison 
with  its  minor  Third.  Thus  in  Handel's  oratorios,  when  he  concludes  with  a 
minor  chord,  most  of  the  conspicuous  vocal  and  instrumental  parts  are  concen- 
trated on  the  tonic,  while  the  minor  Third  is  either  touched  by  one  voice  alone,  or 
merely  by  the  accompanying  pianoforte  or  organ.  The  cases  are  much  rarer  where 
in  minor  keys  he  gives  only  two  voices  to  the  tonic  in  the  closing  chord,  and  one 
to  its  Fifth  and  another  to  its  Third,  which  is  his  rule  in  major  modes.  IT 

When  the  minor  chord  appears  in  its  second  Subordinate  signification,  as 
e^\}+g..x  with  the  root  e^\},  this  (aoi  is  shewn  partly  by  the  position  of  e^\}  in  the 
bass,  and  partly  by  its  close  relationship  to  the  tonic  &^t>.  Modem  music  even 
makes  this  interpretation  of  the  chord  still  clearer  by  adding  Z»^t>  as  the  Fifth  of  e^t>> 
so  that  the  chord  becomes  dissonant  in  the  form  e^l}—g-\-b^\}..,c^.* 

The  disinclination  of  older  composers  to  close  with  a  minor  chord,  may  be 
explained  partly  by  the  obscuration  of  its  consonance  from  false  combinational  tones, 
and  partly  because,  as  already  mentioned,  it  does  not  give  a  mere  quality  of  the 
tonic  tone,  but  mixes  foreign  constituents  with  it.  But  in  addition  to  the  minor 
Third,  which  does  not  fit  into  the  compound  tone  of  the  tonic,  the  combinational 
tones  of  a  minor  chord  are  equally  foreign  to  it.  As  long  as  the  feeling  of  tonaHty 
required  a  definite  single  compound  tone  for  the  connecting  centre  of  the  key,  it 
was  impossible  to  form  a  satisfeu^tory  close  except  by  a  reproduction  of  the  pure 
compound  tone  of  the  tonic  with  no  foreign  admixture.  It  was  not  till  a  farther  ^ 
development  of  musical  feeling  had  given  the  chords  of  the  mode  an  independent 
significance,  that  the  minor  chord,  notwithstanding  its  possession  of  constituents 
foreign  to  the  compound  tone  of  the  tonic,  could  be  justified  in  its  use  as  a  close. 

Hauptmann  f  gives  a  different  reason  for  avoiding  the  minor  chord  at  the  close. 
He  asserts  that  before  the  chord  of  the  dominant  Seventh  came  into  use,  there  was 
no  voice-part  suitable  for  fedling  into  the  minor  Third  of  the  tonic.  Thus  if  the  final 
cadence  consisted  of  the  chords  G+Bi— -D,  C— ^'t>  +  G^,  the  D  of  the  first  chord 
was  the  only  one  which  could  pass  melodiously  in  E^\},  but  this  would  have  ap- 
peared like  the  passage  of  the  leading  note  D  in  the  key  of  E^\}  major  into  its  tonic 
E^\},  and  hence  have  called  up  the  feeling  of  E^\}  major  in  Heu  of  C  minor.  We 
may  admit  that  this  relation  of  the  leading  note  would  have  drawn  the  hearer's 
special  attention  to  the  two  tones  in  question,  and  to  a  certain  extent  disturbed  his 
recognition  of  the  key,  but  yet  it  is  clear  that  even  without  the  help  of  this  chord 
of  the  dominant  Seventh,  there  were  several  ways  for  the  voices  to  pass  through  ^ 
dissonances  into  the  minor  Third  of  the  closing  chord,  if  composers  had  felt  any 
wish  to  do  so.    Thus  in  the  plagal  cadence 

c  -e>|>-^  g    ...& 

0  -  e^\}+  g    ...  cf 

which  is  so  often  used  on  other  occasions,  the  Fourth  /  could  be  made  to  descend 
to  the  minor  Third  e^\}  without  any  inconvenience.    Indeed,  we  find  that  when 

hereafter  drawn  to  this  important  distinction,  Observe  that  it  is  c*  which  is  now  introduced 

see  p.  299a. — Translator,]  in  the  text,  in  place  of  c.    If  c  is  retained, 

*  [Transposing  the  c*  the  chord  becomes  thus  c— e*b  +  <;— 6'b,  the  chord  is  one  of  those 

c*  I  e'b  +  i;— 6' b,  BO  that  we  have  a  major  chord  chords  of  the  Seventh  considered  in  Chapter 

with  the  Sixth  of  its  root  added,  that  is,  the  X\l.- -Translator,] 

snbdominant  of  the  key  of  JB'b  rendered  dis-  f  Harmonik  und  Mclrik,  Leipzig,   1853, 

sonant  by  introducing  c\  the  Second  of  the  p.  216. 
key,  or  the  Sixth  above  the  Bubdominant  c' b.  V^OOQIC 


2  96  DEGREES  OF  RELATIONSHIP  OF  CHORDS.  part  hi. 

the  chord  of  the  dominant  Seventh  had  actually  come  into  use,  and  the  Saventh  F 
of  the  chord  (?+5i  —  Z)  |  F  ought  by  every  right  to  have  descended  into  the  minor 
Third  E^\}  of  the  closing  chord,  musical  pieces  of  the  fifteenth  century*  avoid  this 
progression,  and  make  this  Seventh  F  either  ascend  to  the  Fifth  G,  or  descend  to 
the  major  Third  ^i  of  the  final  chord,  instead  of  to  E^\}y  its  minor  Third.  This 
custom  prevailed  down  to  Bach's  time. 

In  Chapter  XUI.  (p.  249a)  we  characterised  modem  harmonic  music,  as  con- 
trasted with  medieval  polyphony,  by  its  development  of  a  feeling  for  the  independent 
significance  of  chords.  In  Palestrina,  Gabrieli,  and  still  more  in  Monteverde  and  the 
first  composers  of  operas,  we  find  the  various  degrees  of  harmoniousness  in  chords 
carefully  used  for  the  purposes  of  expression.  But  these  masters  are  almost  entirely 
without  any  feeling  for  the  mutual  relation  of  consecutive  chords.  These  chords 
often  follow  one  another  by  entirely  unconnected  leaps,  and  the  only  bond  of  union 
.  II  is  the  scale,  to  which  all  their  notes  belong. 

The  transformation  which  took  place  from  the  sixteenth  to  the  beginning  of  the 
eighteenth  century,  may,  I  think,  be  characterised  by  the  development  of  a  feeling 
for  the  independent  relationship  of  chords  one  to  the  other,  and  by  the  establish- 
ment of  a  central  core,  the  tonic  chordy  round  which  were  grouped  the  whole  of 
the  consonant  chords  that  could  be  formed  out  of  the  notes  of  the  scale.  For  these 
chords  there  was  a  repetition  of  the  same  effort  which  was  formerly  shewn  in  the 
construction  of  the  scale,  where  interrelations  of  the  tones  were  first  grounded  on 
a  chain  of  intervals,  and  afterwards  on  a  reference  of  each  note  to  a  central  com- 
pound tone,  the  tonic. 

Two  chords  which  have  one  or  more  tones  in  common  will  here  be  termed 
directly  related. 

Chords  which  are  directly  related  to  the  same  chord  will  be  here  said  to  be 
related  to  each  other  in  the  second  degree, 
H        Thus  c+Ci  —g  and  g-\'hx—d  are  directly  related,  and  so  are  c+ei—g  and  aj  — 
cH-^i  ;  but  g  +  bi—d  and  ai—c+Ci  are  related  only  in  the  second  degree. 

When  two  chords  have  two  tones  in  common  they  are  more  closely  related  than 
when  they  have  only  one  tone  in  common.  Thus  c+ej  —  ^  and  Ui  — c+ei  are  more 
closely  related  than  c  -f-  e  1  —  gf  and  gf  -h  6 ,  —  d. 

The  tonic  chord  of  any  tonal  mode  can  of  course  only  be  one  which  more  or 
less  perfectly  represents  the  compound  tone  of  the  tonic,  that  is,  that  major  or 
minor  chord  of  which  the  tonic  is  the  root.  The  tonic  note,  as  the  connecting  core 
of  all  the  tones  in  a  regularly  constructed  melody,  must  be  heard  on  the  first 
accented  part  of  a  bar,  and  also  at  the  close,  so  that  the  melody  starts  from  it  and 
returns  to  it ;  the  same  is  true  for  the  tonic  chord  in  a  succession  of  chords.  In 
both  of  these  positions  in  the  scale  we  require  to  hear  the  tonic  note,  accompanied 
not  by  any  arbitrary  chord,  but  only  by  the  tonic  chord,  having  the  tonic  note 
itself  as  its  root.  This  was  not  the  case  even  as  late  as  the  sixteenth  century,  as 
1[  is  seen  by  the  example  on  p.  247c  taken  from  Palestrina. 

When  the  tonic  chord  is  major,  the  domination  of  all  the  tones  by  the  tonic 
rote  is  readily  reconciled  with  the  domination  of  all  the  chords  by  the  tonic  chord* 
for  as  the  piece  begins  and  ends  with  the  tonic  chord,  it  also  begins  and  ends  at  the 
same  time  with  the  pure  unmixed  compound  tone  of  the  tonic  note.  But  when 
the  tonic  chord  is  minor,  all  these  conditions  cannot  be  so  perfectly  satisfied.  We 
are  obliged  to  sacrifice  somewhat  of  the  strictness  of  the  tonality  in  order  to  admit 
the  minor  Third  into  the  tonic  chord  at  the  beginning  and  end.  At  the  com- 
mencement of  the  eighteenth  century  we  find  Sebastian  Bach  using  minor  chords 
at  the  end  of  his  preludes,  because  these  were  merely  introductory  pieces,  but  not 
at  the  end  of  fugues  and  chorales,  and  at  other  complete  closes.  In  Handel  and  even 
in  the  ecclesiastical  pieces  of  Mozart,  the  close  in  a  minor  chord  is  used  alternately 

•  See  an  example  in  Anton  Brnmel,  in  will  be  found,  iMd,  p.  550,  where  the  voices 
Forkers  Oeschichte  der  Musikf  vol.  ii.  p.  647.  might  have  easily  been  led  to  the  minor 
Another,  with  a  plagal  cadence  by  Josquin,      Third. 

Digitized  by  VjOOQlC 


CHAP.  XV.  MINOR  CHORD  IN  THE   CLOSE.  29? 

with  the  close  in  a  chord  without  any  Third,  or  with  the  major  Third.  And  the 
last  composer  cannot  be  accused  of  external  imitation  of  old  habits,  for  we  find 
that  in  these  usages  they  always  observe  the  expression  of  the  piece.  When  at  the 
close  of  a  composition  in  the  minor  mode,  a  major  chord  is  introduced,,  it  has  the 
effect  of  a  sudden  and  unexpected  brightening  up  of  the  sadness  of  the  minor  key, 
producing  a  cheering,  enlightening,  and  reconciling  effect  after  the  sorrow,  grief, 
or  restlessness  of  the  minor.  Thus  a  close  in  the  major  suits  the  prayer  for  the 
peace  of  the  departed  in  the  words,  *  et  lux  perpetua  luceat  eis,'  or  the  conclusion 
of  the  Confutatis  Tnaledictis,  which  runs  thus : — 

Oro  Bupplex  et  acclinis, 
Cor  contritum  quasi  ciuis ; 
Gere  coram  mei  finis. 

But  such  a  closing  major  chord  is  certainly  somewhat  startling  for  our  present  f 
musical  feeling,  even  though  its  introduction  may,  at  one  time,  add  wondrous 
beauty  and  solemnity,  or,  at  another,  dart  like  a  beam  of  hope  into  the  gloom 
of  deepest  despair.  If  the  restlessness  remains  to  the  last,  as  in  the  Dies  irae  of 
Mozart's  Bequiem,  the  minor  chord,  in  which  an  unresolved  disturbance  exists, 
forms  a  fitting  close.  Mozart  was  wont  to  terminate  ecclesiastical  pieces  of  a  less 
decided  character  with  a  chord  that  had  no  Third.  There  are  many  similar 
examples  in  Handel.  Hence  although  both  masters  stood  on  the  very  same  plat- 
form as  modem  musical  feeling,  and  themselves  gave,  as  it  were,  the  finishing 
touch  to  the  construction  of  the  modem  tonal  system,  they  were  not  altogether 
strangers  to  the  feeling  which  had  prevented  older  musicians  from  using  the  minor 
Third  of  the  tonic  in  the  final  chord.  They  followed  no  strict  rule,  however,  but 
acted  according  to  the  expression  and  characteir  of  the  piece  and  the  sense  of  the 
words  with  which  they  had  to  close. 

Those  tonal  modes  which  furnish  the  greatest  number  of  consonant  chords  «r 
related  to  one  another  or  to  the  common  chord,  are  best  adapted  for  artistically 
connected  harmonies.  Since  all  consonant  chords,  when  reduced  to  their  closest 
position  and  simplest  form,  are  triads  consisting  of  a  major  and  a  minor  Third,  all 
the  consonant  chords  of  any  key  can  be  found  by  simply  arranging  them  in  order 
of  Thirds,  as  in  the  following  tables.  The  braces  above  and  below  connect  the 
chords  together.  The  ordinary  round  braces,  which  are  placed  above,  point  out 
minor  chords ;  the  square  braces  below  indicate  major  chorda.  The  tonic  chord 
is  printed  in  capitals. 

i)  Majob  mode 


di  -  f  +  ai  —  C  ±  El  -  G  +  bi  --  d 

I 1 1 !  I ! 

2)  Mode  of  the  Foubth 


6l>+  dt  -f  +  at  -  C  +  Et-  G  -  i'|?+  d 


3)  Mode  of  the  hinob  Seventh 


b\>+  di-f  +  at-  C  -  £'t>+  a  -  6'1>+  d 

I  II 


J         I I 


4)  Mode  of  the  minob  Thibd 


I I      ! ^1 ^1 


5)  Mode  of  the  uinob  Sixth 

-E'\}+  G- 

1 1  _ 

Digitized  by  VjOOQlC 


Jl>-i't)  +  /-o'|?+  C  -  E'\}+  G-  i'l) 

I !  I  II  I 


298  INTONATION  OP  THE  INTEBCALABY  TONES.        pabt  m. 

In  this  ajrangement  I  have  introduced  the  different  intonations  of  the  Second 
and  Seventh  of  the  key,  which  we  found  in  the  construction  of  the  scales  for 
homophonic  music*  But  we  observe  that  the  chords  directly  related  to  the  tonio 
chord  contain  every  tone  in  the  scale,  excepting  in  the  mode  of  the  minor  Sixth. 
The  Second  and  Seventh  of  the  tonic  occur  first  in  the  chord  of  G,  which  is  directly 
related  to  the  tonic  chord,  and  next  in  chords  containing  jP,  which  are,  however,  not 
directly  connected  with  the  tonic  chord.  The  supplementary  tones  of  the  scale 
which  are  related  to  the  dominant  thus  acquire  in  harmonic  music  an  important 
preponderance  over  those  related  to  the  subdominant.  We  must  necessarily  prefer 
direct  to  indirect  relations  for  determining  scalar  degrees.  Hence  by  confining 
ourselves  to  the  chords  which  are  directly  related  to  the  tonic  chord,  we  obtain  the 
following  arrangement  of  the  tonal  modes  :t — 

f       i)  Major  mode 


f+ai-C+Ei-G+bi-d 
I II II I 


2)  Mode  of  the  Foubth 


I II I 


3)  Mode  op  the  minob  Seventh 


/+tt,-C-B»t>  +  (?-6't>+i 


4)  Mode  of  the  hinob  Thibo 


V  I 11 I 

5)  Mods  of  the  minob  Sixth 


d'\>+f^a'\>+C-E'\}-hG-b% 


A  glance  at  this  table  shews  that  the  major  mode  and  mode  of  the  minor  Third 
(minor  mode)  possess  the  most  complete  and  connected  series  of  chords,  so  that 
these  two  are  decidedly  superior  to  the  rest  for  harmonic  purposes.  This  is  also 
the  reason  which  led  to  the  preference  given  to  them  in  modem  music. 

And  in  this  way  we  obtain  a  final  settlement  of  the  proper  intonation  of  the 
supplementary  tones  of  the  scale,  at  least  for  the  first  four  modes.  Hauptmann, 
with  whom  I  agree,  considers  the  tone  D  alone  to  be  the  essential  constituent  of 
both  the  major  and  minor  modes  of  G.  This  D  forms  an  imperfect  (Pythagorean) 
H  minor  Third  with  F^  so  that  the  chord  D  \  F-k-A^  must  be  considered  as  dissonant.^ 
This  chord  thus  intoned  is  in  reaHty  most  decidedly  dissonant  to  the  ear.  On  the 
other  hand,  Hauptmann  admits  a  major  mode  which  reaches  over  to  the  sub- 
dominant,  and  uses  Dx  in  place  of  D.    I  consider  this  conception  to  be  a  very 

*  [These  soales  differ  from  those  tran-  the  double  modality  alluded  to  in  the  last 
scribed  in  pp.  293^  and  294a,  only  in  the  addi-      note,  and  fixes  the  modes  in  the  meanings 


tion  of  the  secondary  forms  of  intercalary  tones,      of  App.  XX.  sect.  E.  art.  9,  as 
dp  bbt  or  6^b,  which,  in  fact,  imply  modula-  (i)  i  C  ma.ma.ma. 

tions    into    adjacent    modes,    or    else    give  (2)  i  C  ma.ma.mL 


dp  bbt  or  6^b,  which,  in  fact,  imply  modula-  (i)  i  C  ma.ma.ma. 

tions    into    adjacent    modes,    or    else    give  (2)  i  C  ma.ma.mL 

a  double  and  ambiguous  character  to  each  (3)  i  C  majm.mi. 


mode,  as  shewn  on  p.  277,  footnote  f,  and  by  (4)  i  C  mi.mi.mi. 

referring  to  the  Duodenarium,  App.  XX.  sect.  (5)  3  ^'bma.ma.ma. 

£.  art.  18,  it  will  be  seen  that  there  is  a  real  In  the  last  scale  it  is  more  usual,  however,  to 

change  of  duodene,  which  always  must  happen  take  6  b  in  place  of  6'b»  which  makes  the  scale 

when  changes  of  a  comma   occur. — Trans-  6b-d*b+/— o'b  +  C— ^'b +0«5Fmijnijiii. 

latorJ]  But  temperament  obscures  all  these  dififer- 

t  [The  first  four  are  the  same  as  in  pp.  zg^d  ences.—TraTtslator.] 
and  294^.    The  settlement  in  the  text  avoids  \  [See  p.  295(i,  note  *. — Traiislaior.] 


Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


CHAP.  XV.  HARMONIC  DIFFERENCE  BETWEEN  MAJOR  AND  MINOR.  299 

happy  expression  of  the  real  state  of  things.  When  the  consonant  chord 
Di—F+Ai  occurs  in  any  composition  it  is  impossible  to  return  immediately, 
without  any  transitional  tone,  to  the  tonic  chord  C-^E^  —  G,  The  result  would 
be  felt  as  an  harmonic  leap  without  adequate  notice.  Hence  it  is  a  correct 
expression  of  the  state  of  affairs  to  look  upon  the  use  of  this  chord  as  the 
beginning  of  a  modulation  beyond  the  boundaries  of  the  key  of  G  major,  that  is, 
beyond  the  limits  of  direct  relationship  to  its  tonic  chord.  In  the  minor  mode 
this  would  correspond  to  a  modulation  into  the  chord  of  D^\}+F—A^\}.  Of  course 
in  the  modem  tempered  intonation  the  consonant  chord  Di—F+Ai  is  not  dis- 
tinguished from  the  dissonant  D  |  F+Ai,  and  hence  the  feeling  of  musicians  has 
not  been  sufficiently  cultivated  to  make  them  appreciate  this  difference  on  which 
Hauptmann  insists.* 

As  regards  the  other  supplementary  tone  b^\},  which  may  occur  in  the  chords 
e'b+gf— ^*[>  SiJidg  —  b^\}-^d\  1  have  already  shewn  in  the  last  chapter  that  even  in  ^ 
homophonic  music  it  is  almost  always  replaced  by  &i.  Harmonic  considerations 
likewise  favour  the  use  of  b^  independently  of  melodic  progression.  It  has  been 
already  shewn  that  when  the  two  tones  of  the  scale  which  are  but  distantly 
related  to  the  tonic,  make  their  appearance  as  constituents  of  the  dominant,  they 
enter  into  close  relation  to  the  tonic.  Now  this  can  only  be  the  case  with  the 
compound  tones  of  the  major  chord  g+bi—d,  and  not  with  those  of  the  minor 
chord  g^b^\}+d»  Considered  independently,  the  tones  b^\}  and  d  are  quite  as 
closely  related  to  c  as  the  tones  b^  and  d.  But  by  regarding  the  two  latter  as 
constituents  of  the  compound  tone  g,  we  connect  them  with  c  by  the  same 
closeness  of  relationship  that  g  is  itself  connected  with  c.  Hence,  in  all  modem 
music,  wherever  b^\}  might  occur  as  a  constituent  of  the  dominant  chord  of  the  key 
of  0  minor,  or  of  some  dissonant  chord  replacing  the  dominant  chord,  it  is  usual  to 
change  it  into  bi  and  otherwise  to  use  either  b^\}OT  bi  according  to  the  melodic 
progression,  but  more  frequently  the  latter,  as  I  have  already  remarked  when  ^ 
treating  of  the  construction  of  minor  scales.  It  is  this  systematic  use  of  the  major 
Seventh  bi  in  place  of  the  minor  Seventh  5^t>  of  the  key  which  now  distinguishes 
the  modem  minor  mode  from  the  ancient  Hypodoric,t  or  the  mode  of  the  minor 
Third.  Here  again  some  part  of  the  consistency  of  the  scale  is  sacrificed  in 
order  to  bind  the  harmony  closer  together. 

The  chain  of  consonant  chords  in  the  mode  of  the  minor  Third  is  certainly 
impaired  when  that  mode  is  transformed  into  our  minor  by  the  introduction  oibi. 
In  place  of  the  chain 


f^a'\>+C'-E'\>+G-b^M 
our  minor  furnishes  only 


I I        J I 

which  has  one  triad  less.    But  the  composer  is  at  liberty  to  alternate  the  two 
tones  b^\}  and  bi. 

The  introduction  of  the  leading  note  5i  into  the  key  of  c  minor  generated  a  new 
difficulty  in  the  complete  closing  cadence  of  this  key.  When  the  chord  g  +  bi—d 
is  followed  by  the  chord  c—e*b+gr,  the  first  being  a  perfectly  harmonious  major  chord, 
and  the  latter  an  obscurely  harmonious  minor  chord,  the  defect  in  the  harmonious- 

*  [This  was  referred  to  in  p.  2g4d,  note  §.  be  traced  on  the  Duodenarinm. — Translator.'] 
See  App.  XX.  sect.  £.  art.  26,  example  of  the  f  [Hyi)odoric,  also  caUed  Eolio,  p.  26Sd,  foot- 

uae  of  duodenals.    It  is  a  real,  though  tern-  note  No.  6,  but  here  the  harmonic  alteration 

porary  modulation  into  a  new  duodene,  one  of  that  mode  is  meant  as  in  p.  274,  footnote 

Fifth  lower.    But  for  D  |  F+il,  we  might  use  No.  3.    This  confusion  is  here  regular  and  in- 

D—F^  +  A,  which  is  again  a  modulation  into  ienUoB&l.^TransUUor,] 
a  new  duodene,  one  Fifth  higher.    This  should 


Digitized  by 


Google 


300    HARMONIC  DIFFERENCE  BETWEEN  MAJOR  AND  MINOR,  pabtiik 

ness  of  the  latter  is  made  much  more  e\ddent  by  the  contrast.  But  it  is  precisely 
in  the  final  chord  that  perfect  consonance  is  essential  to  satisfy  the  feeling  of  the 
hearer.  Hence  this  close  could  not  become  satisfactory  until  the  chord  of  the 
dominant  Seventh  had  been  invented,  which  changed  the  dominant  consonance 
into  a  dissonance. 

The  preceding  explanation  shews  that  when  we  try  to  institute  a  close  con- 
nection among  all  the  chords  peculiar  to  a  mode  similar  to  the  close  connection 
among  the  tones  of  the  scale,  (that  is,  when  we  require  all  the  consonant  triads 
in  the  harmonic  tissue  to  be  related  to  one  of  tlieir  number,  the  tonic  triad,  in  a 
manner  analogous  to  that  in  which  the  notes  of  the  scale  are  related  to  one  of 
their  number,  the  tonic  tone),  there  are  only  two  tonal  modes,  the  major  and 
wiTior,  which  properly  satisfy  such  conditions  of  related  tones  and  related  chords. 

The  major  mode  fulfils  the  two  conditions  of  chordal  relationship  and  tonal 
II  relationship  in  the  most  perfect  manner.  It  has  four  triads  which  are  immediately 
related  to  tlie  tonic  chord 


L II II I 

Its  harmonisation  can  be  so  conducted,  (indeed,  in  popular  pieces  which  must  be 
readily  inteUigible,  it  is  so  conducted,)  that  all  tones  appear  as  constituents  of  the 
three  major  chords  of  the  system,  those  of  the  tonic,  dominant,  and  subdominant. 
These  major  chords,  when  their  roots  lie  lo^,  appear  to  the  ear  as  reinforcements 
of  the  compound  tones  of  the  tonic,  dominant,  and  subdominant,  which  tones  are 
themselves  connected  by  the  closest  possible  relationship  of  Fifths.  Hence  in 
this  mode  everything  can  be  reduced  to  the  closest  musical  relationship  in  existence. 
And  since  the  tonic  chord  in  this  case  represents  the  compound  tone  of  the  tonic 
%  immediately  and  completely,  the  two  conditions —predominance  of  the  tonic  tone 
and  of  the  tonic  chord — go  hand  in  hand,  without  the  possibility  of  any  contra- 
diction, or  the  necessity  of  making  any  changes  in  the  scale. 

The  major  mode  has,  therefore,  the  character  of  possessing  the  most  complete 
melodic  and  harmonic  consistency,  combined  with  the  greatest  simplicity  and 
clearness  in  all  its  relations.  Moreover,  its  predominant  chords  being  major,  are 
distinguished  by  full  unobscured  harmoniousness,  when  such  positions  are  selected 
for  them  as  do  not  introduce  inappropriate  combinational  tones. 

The  major  scale  is  purely  diatonic,  and  possesses  the  ascending  leading  note  of 
the  major  Seventh,  whence  it  results  that  the  tone  most  distantly  related  to  the 
tonic  is  brought  into  closest  melodic  connection  with  it. 

The  three  predominant  major  chords  furnish  tones  sufficient  to  produce  two 
minor  chords,  which  are  closely  related  to  them,  and  can  be  employed  to  diversify 
the  succession  of  major  chords. 
i)        The  minor  mode  is  in  many  respects  inferior  to  the  major.   The  chain  of  chords 
for  its  modem  form  is — 


/-a»b+C-^^»b+  G+h.-d 


Elinor  chords  do  not  represent  the  compound  tone  of  their  root  as  well  as  the 
major  chords  ;  their  Third,  indeed,  does  not  form  any  part  of  this  compound  tone. 
The  dominant  chord  alone*  is  major,  and  it  contains  the  two  supplementary  tones  of 
the  scale.  Hence  when  these  appear  as  constituents  of  the  dominant  triad,  and 
therefore  of  the  compound  tone  of  the  dominant,  they  are  connected  with  the  tonic 
by  the  close  relationship  of  Fifths.  On  the  other  hand,  the  tonic  and  subdominant 
triads  do  not  simply  represent  the  compound  tones  of  the  tonic  and  subdominant 
notes,  but  are  accompanied  by  Thirds  which  cannot  be  reduced  to  the  close  relation- 

*  [That  is,  among  the  characteristic  chords,  textf  contain  the  tones  of  one  major  chord, 
The  two  minor  chords,  as  is  8he>»'n  in  the      a'b +c-c'b.— Transiator.] 

Digitized  by  V^OOQIC 


CHAP.  XV.  HARMONIC  DIFFERENCE  BETWEEN  MAJOR  AND  MINOR.  301 

ship  of  Fifths.  The  tones  of  the  minor  scale  can  therefore  not  be  harmonised  in 
such  a  way  as  to  link  them  with  the  tonic  note  by  so  close  a  relationship  as  in  the 
major  mode. 

The  conditions  of  tonality  cannot  be  so  simply  reconciled  with  the  predominance 
of  the  tonic  chord  as  in  the  major  mode.  When  a  piece  concludes  with  a  minor 
chord,  we  hear,  in  addition  to  the  compound  tone  of  the  tonic  note,  a  second 
compound  tone  which  is  not  a  constituent  of  the  first.  This  accounts  for  the  long 
hesitation  of  musical  composers  respecting  the  admissibility  of  a  minor  chord  in 
the  close. 

The  predominant  minor  chords  have  not  the  clearness  and  unobscured  har- 
moniousness  of  the  major  chords,  because  they  are  accompanied  by  combinational 
tones  which  do  not  fit  into  the  chord. 

The  minor  scale  contains  an  interval  a^b...6i,  which  exceeds  a  whole  Tone  in 
the  diatonic  scale,*  and  answers  to  the  numerical  ratio  75  :  64  [=274  cents].  ^ 
To  make  the  minor  scale  melodic  it  must  have  a  different  form  in  descending  from 
what  it  has  in  ascending,  as  mentioned  in  the  last  chapter. 

The  minor  mode,  therefore,  has  no  such  simple,  clear,  intelligible  consistency  as 
the  major  mode ;  it  has  arisen,  as  it  were,  from  a  compromise  between  the  different 
conditions  of  the  laws  of  tonality  and  the  interlinking  of  harmonies.  Hencfe  it  is 
also  much  more  variable,  much  more  inclined  to  modulations  into  other  modes. 

This  assertion  that  the  minor  system  is  much  less  consistent  than  the  major, 
win  be  combated  by  many  modem  musicians,  just  as  they  have  contested  the 
assertion  already  made  by  me,  and  by  other  physicists  before  me,  that  minor  triads 
are  generally  inferior  in  harmoniousness  to  major  triads.  There  are  many  eager 
assurances  of  the  contrary  in  recent  books  on  the  theory  of  harmony.f  But  the 
history  of  music,  the  extremely  slow  and  careful  development  of  the  minor  system 
in  the  sixteenth  and  seventeenth  centuries,  the  guarded  use  of  the  minor  close  by 
Handel,  the  partial  avoidance  of  a  minor  close  even  by  Mozart, — all  these  seem  to  ^ 
leave  no  doubt  that  the  artistic  feeling  of  the  great  composers  agreed  with  our 
conclusions.^  To  this  must  be  added  the  varied  use  of  the  major  and  minor 
Seventh,  and  the  major  and  minor  Sixth  of  the  scale,  the  modulations  rapidly 
introduced  and  rapidly  changing,  and  finally,  but  very  decisively,  popular  custom. 
Popular  melodies  can  contain  none  but  clear  transparent  relations.  Look  through 
collections  of  songs  now  preferred  by  those  classes  among  the  Western  nations  which 
have  often  an  opportunity  of  hearing  harmonic  music,  as  students,  soldiers, 
artisans.  There  are  scarcely  one  or  two  per  cent,  in  minor  keys,  and  those  are 
mostly  old  popular  songs  which  have  descended  from  the  times  of  homophonic 
music.    It  is  also  characteristic  that,  as  I  have  been  assured  by  an  experienced 

♦  [The  interval  ie  so  strange,  when  nnaccom-  falling  upon  the  same  note  with  which  they 

panied,  that  if  it  had  to  be  taken  merely  as  an  began,  will  take  e\^  the  major  Third  of  c'. 

interval,  a*  b  2745,,  a  singer  would  probably  fail.  Hence  the  difficulty  is  not  avoided  but  in- 

But  the  a>  b  is  taken  as  the  minor  Third  of  /  creased  by  introducing  the  ambiguity  of  the  ^ 

with  ease,  and  the  6,  is  taken  as  the  leading  major  key,  into  which  this  is  a  real  modulation 

note  to  c',  with  6qual  ease,  so  that  the  per-  from  q  onwards. — Translator.] 
f ectly  unmelodic  and  inharmonic  interval  a'  b  f  [Can  this  be  due  to  temperament  ?    The 

274&,  never  comes  into  consideration  at  all.  sharp  equally  tempered  major  Third  of  400 

To  get  rid  of  it,  the  subdominant  is  often  taken  cents  is  worse  of  its  kind  than  the  flat  equally 

major,  producing  the  chords  of  i  C  ma.mi.ma.,  tempered  minor  Third  of  300  cents,  which 

App.  XX.  sect.  E.  art.  10,  III.,  which  makes  the  approaches  close  to   16:19-298    cents,   an 

Bcalec204dii2e'bi82/204gi82a,2046,ii2c',  interval  which  many  like,  and  which  maybe 

and  this  differs  from  lie  major  only  by  having  tried  as  ef"  I  -V'b  on  the  Harmonical. — Trans- 

e^b  in  place  of  e^    In  many  pianoforte  in-  lator.] 

stmction  books  this  is  given  as  the  only  form  {  [These  composers  played  in  meantone 

of  tl  e  ascending  minor.  Mr.  Curwen  (Standard  temperament  (App.  XX.  sect.  A.  art.  16),  in 

Course,  p.  86)  says  that    this    major   Sixth  which  the  minor  Third  of  310  cents  was  much 

*  ascending  is  very  difficult  to  sing,*  and  *  has  rougher  than  the  equally  tempered  one  of  300 

a  hard  and  by  no  means  pleasant  effect,*  and  cents,  having  much   slower  beats.    Possibly 

points  out  that  it  leads  singers  to  forget  the  this  difference  in  the  modes  of  tempering  the 

key,  and  in  such  a  phrase  as  g  a,  6,  c'  d'  c"  b,  the  minor  Third,  may  have  led  to  the  difference  of 

pupils  will  sing  e\  instead  of  e'*b;  and  even  in  opinion  mentioned  in  the  text.— Tf aiwtotor.l 


singing  such  a  passage  as  ^  a,  6,  c'  g,  instead  of 


Digitized  by  V^OOQIC 


302 


HAKMONISATION  OF  THE  TONAL  MODES. 


PABT  in. 


teacher  of  singing,  pupils  of  only  moderate  musical  talent  have  much  more  difficulty 
in  hitting  the  minor  than  the  major  Third. 

But  I  am  by  no  means  of  opinion  that  this  character  depreciates  the  minor 
system.  The  major  mode  is  well  suited  for  all  frames  of  mind  which  are  completely 
formed  and  clearly  understood,  for  strong  resolve,  and  for  soft  and  gentle  or  even 
for  sorrowing  feelings,  when  the  sorrow  has  passed  into  the  condition  of  dreamy 
and  yielding  regret.  But  it  is  quite  unsuited  for  indistinct,  obscure,  unformed 
frames  of  mind,  or  for  the  expression  of  the  dismal,  the  dreary,  the  enigmatic,  the 
mysterious,  the  rude,  and  whatever  offends  against  artistic  beauty  ; — and  it  is  pre- 
cisely for  these  that  we  require  the  minor  mode,  with  its  veiled  harmoniousness, 
its  changeable  scale,  its  ready  modulation,  and  less  intelligible  basis  of  construc- 
tion. The  major  mode  would  be  an  unsuitable  form  for  such  purposes,  and 
hence  the  minor  mode  has  its  own  proper  artistic  justification  as  a  separate 
f  system. 

The  harmonic  peculiarities  of  the  modem  keys  are  best  seen  by  comparing 
them  with  the  harmonisation  of  the  other  ancient  tonal  modes. 

Major  Modb. 

Among  the  melodic  tonal  modes  the  Lydian  of  the  Greeks  (the  ecclesiastical 
Ionic  [p.  274,  note,  No.  i] ),  in  agreement  with  our  major,  is  the  only  one  which  has 
an  ascending  leading  note  in  the  form  of  a  major  Seventh.  The  four  others  had 
originally  and  naturally  only  minor  Sevenths,  which  even  in  the  later  periods  of 
the  middle  ages  began  to  give  place  to  major  Sevenths,  in  order  that  the  Seventh 
of  the  scale,  which  was  in  itself  so  loosely  connected  with  the  tonic,  might  be  more 
closely  united  to  it  by  becoming  the  leading  note  to  the  tonic  at  the  close. 

Mode  op  the  Fourth. 

^  The  inode  of  the  Fourth  (the  Greek  Ionic,  and  ecclesiastical  Mixolydian)  is 
principally  distinguished  from  the  major  mode  by  its  minor  Seventh.  By  merely 
changing  this  into  the  major  we  obliterate  the  difference  between  them.  Taking 
C  as  the  tonic  the  chain  of  chords  in  the  unaltered  mode  are  as  on  p.  2986,  No.  2, 


I II I 

If  we  attempt  to  form  a  complete  cadence  in  this  mode,  as  in  the  following 
examples  i  and  2,  they  will  sound  dull  from  want  of  the  leading  note,  even  when 
the  dominant  chord  is  extended  to  a  chord  of  the  Seventh  g—b^\}+d\f,  as  in  2. 


'I 


i[C]* 


2[C] 


3[C] 


fM=j=ii 


\fS       fJ-:$^;=si 


E^^ 


U 


^EE 


^ 


*  I 

I 


^ 


ite 


::tsi 


The  second  example,  in  which  the  leading  note  li^^  lies  uppermost,  is  even  duller 
than  the  first  example,  in  which  that  note  Z^^[>ismore  concealed.  The  i^^in  these 
examples  has  a  very  uncertain  sound.  It  is  not  closely  enough  related  to  the 
tonic,  it  is  not  part  of  the  compound  tone  of  the  dominant  note  g,  it  is  not  suffi- 
ciently close  in  pitch  to  serve  as  a  leading  note  to  the  tonic,  and  it  has  no  tendency 


*  [The  [0]  ifl  the  duodenal  of  App.  XX. 
sect.  E.  art.  26,  shewing  the  exact  pitch  of  all 
the  notea.    These  examples  have  been  trans- 


posed to  admit  of  their  being  played  on  the 
Harmonioal, — Translator,] 


Digitized  by 


Google 


CHAP.  XV.        HARMONISATION  OP  THE  TONAL  MODES-  303 

to  push  on  to  the  tonic.  Hence  when  the  older  composers  wished  to  distinguish 
pieces  written  in  the  mode  of  the  Fourth  from  those  in  the  major  mode,  by  their 
close,  they  employed  the  imperfect  or  plagal  cadence,  as  in  example  3.  And  as 
such  a  cadence  wants  the  decisive  progression  required  for  a  close,  the  sluggishness 
previously  caused  by  the  absence  of  a  leading  tone  ceases  to  be  striking.* 

In  the  course  of  a  piece  written  in  this  tonal  mode,  the  leading  note  bi  may  of 
course  be  used  in  ascending  passages,  provided  the  minor  Seventh  b^\}  is  employed 
often  enough  in  descending  passages.  But  the  effect  of  the  mode  is  destroyed 
when  an  essential  tone  of  the  scale  is  changed  at  the  close.  Hence  pieces  in  the 
mode  of  the  Fourth  sound  like  pieces  in  a  major  mode  which  have  a  decided  incli- 
nation to  modulate  into  the  major  mode  of  the  subdominant.f    For  reasons 

>  already  given,  transition  to  the  subdominant  appears  to  be  less  active  than  transi- 
tion to  the  dominant.  This  tonal  mode  has  also  no  decided  progression  at  the 
close,  whereas  major  chords,  of  which  the  tonic  is  one,  predominate  in  it  owing  to  ^ 
their  greater  harmoniousness.  The  mode  of  the  Fourth  is  consequently  as  soft 
and  harmonious  as  the  major  mode,  but  it  wants  the  powerful  forward  impetus  of 
major  movement.  This  agrees  with  the  character  assigned  to  it  by  Winterfeld.t 
He  describes  the  ecclesiastical  Ionic  (major)  mode,  as  a  scale  which,  '  strictly  self- 
contained  and  founded  on  the  clear  and  bright  major  triad— a  naturally  harmonious 
and  satisfactory  fusion  of  different  tones, — also  bears  the  stamp  of  bright  and 
cheerful  satisfsustion.'  On  the  other  hand,  the  ecclesiastical  Mixolydian  (mode  of 
the  Fourth)  is  a  scale  '  in  which  every  part  by  sound  and  movement  hastens  to  the 
source  of  its  fundamental  tone '  (that  is,  to  the  major  mode  of  its  subdominant), 
'  and  this  gives  it  a  yearning  character  in  addition  to  the  former  cheerful  satisfaction, 
not  unlike  to  the  Christian  yearning  for  spiritual  regeneration  and  redemption,  and 
return  of  primitive  innocency,  though  softened  by  the  bliss  of  love  and  faith'.' 


Mode  of  the  minob  Seventh. 

The  mode  of  the  minor  Seventh  (Greek  Phrygian  [p.  274,  note,  No.  4]  eccle- 
siastical Doric)  has  a  minor  chord  on  c  as  the  tonic,  and  originally  another  on  g 
as  the  dominant,  while  it  has  a  major  chord  on  its  subdominant  /,  and  this  last 
chord  distinguishes  the  mode  from  the  w/>de  of  the  minor  Third  (Eolic  [p.  294(2, 
note,  No.  3] ) ;  thus 

I !    I I 

Both  of  these  modes  of  the  minor  Seventh  and  minor  Third  may,  without 
destroying  their  character,  change  the  minor  Seventh  h^\^  into  a  leading  note  6,, 
and  our  minor  mode  is  a  fusion  of  both.  The  ascending  minor  scale  belongs 
to  the  mode  of  the  minor  Seventh,  in  which  the  leading  note  is  used,  and  the  ^ 
descending  to  the  mode  of  the  minor  Third.  But  when  the  mode  of  the  minor 
Seventh  admits  the  leading  note,  its  chain  of  chords  reduces  to  the  three  essential 
chords  of  the  scale 


This  tonal  mode  has  all  the  character  of  a  minor,  but  the  transition  to  the  chord 
of  the  subdominant  has  a  brighter  effect  than  in  the  normal  minor,  where  the  sub- 

*  [These  can  be  played  on  the  Harmonical.  with    its   subdominant  fga^hbcfd\6\f. — 

—Translator.]  Translator,] 

t  [This  inolination  seems  to  arise  from  the  X  Johannes  GabrieK  und  ssin  Zeitalter, 

tempered  confusion  of  h^\>d  with  &b  (2,  so  that  vol.  i.  p.  87. 
the  scale  c(ie|/(^aj  d'bc'  becomes  confused  ^-^  ^ 

Digitized  by  VjOOQIC 


3^4 


HAEMONISATION  OF  THE  TONAL  MODES. 


PABT    III. 


dominant  chord  is  also  minor.  On  forming  the  complete  cadence  both  dominant 
and  subdominant  chords  are  major,  while  the  tonic  remains  minor.  This  has  of 
course  an  mipleasant  effect  in  the  close,  because  it  makes  the  final  chord  obscurer 
than  either  of  the  other  two  principal  chords.  Hence  it  is  necessary  to  introduce 
strong  dissonances  into  these  two  chords,  to  restore  the  balance.  But  if  we  follow 
the  old  composers  and  make  the  final  chord  major,  we  give  the  closing  cadence  of 
this  mode  an  unmistakably  major  character.  As  in  ecclesiastical  modes  it  is 
always  allowed  to  change  Ai  into  A^\},  which  would  change  the  subdominant 
chord  of  the  mode  of  the  minor  Seventh  into  a  minor  chord,*  we  can  protect  the 
mode  of  the  minor  Seventh  from  confusion  with  the  major  mode  in  its  final 
cadence,  but  then  again  it  will  entirely  coincide  with  the  old  minor  cadence. 

Sebastian  Bach  introduces  the  major  Sixth  of  the  tonic,  which  is  peculiar  to 
this  tonal  mode,  into  other  chords  for  the  closing  cadence,  and  thus  avoids  the 
f  major  triad  on  the  subdominant.  He  very  usually  employs  the  major  Sixth  as  the 
Fifth  of  the  chord  of  the  Seventh  on  the  Second  of  the  scale,t  as  in  the  following 
examples.  No.  i  is  the  conclusion  of  the  chorale :  Was  mein  Gott  loill,  das 
gescheh'  allzeit,  in  the  St.  Matthew  Passion-Music.  No.  2  is  the  conclusion  of  the 
hymn  Veni  redemptor  gentium,  at  the  end  of  the  cantata :  Schwingt  frevdig  Euch 
empor  zu  den  erhabenen  Sternen,    In  both  the  tonic  is  bi,  the  major  Sixth  g^-t 


*  [In  the  original  the  scale  was  g  +  b^  —  D 
—  F^  +  A  +  C|Jl  -e  in  order  that  it  might  run 
from  D  to  d;  and  hence  the  statement  was 
that  it  is  allowable  to  change  B  into  B'b. 
But  in  order  to  keep  to  the  same  notes  as 
were  used  previously,  and  to  allow  of  the 
scale  being  played  on  the  Harmonical,  I  have 
transposed  it,  and  hence  have  had  to  make 
the  same  change  here.  The  result  is  precisely 
the  same,  merely  meaning  that  the  Seventh 
might  be  taken  minor. — Translator.] 

t  [In  the  scale  / +  a,-c-e*b  +  Sf  +  6, -<^, 

a,  is  the  major  Sixth  of  the  tonic  c  and  d  the 

Second.    The  chord  of  the  Seventh  on  the 

^  Second  of  the  scale  is  therefore  d  +/ilf  —  a  |  c. 


hence  if  S.  Bach  makes  this  Fifth  a  agree  with 
the  major  Sixth  of  the  scale  a„  he  is  thinking 
in  tempered  music.  When  just  intonation  is 
restored,  this  occasions  a  restless  modulation 
as  shewn  by  the  duodenals  which  I  hav«  in- 
troduced  over  the  following  examples. — Trotis- 
lator.] 

t  [The  notes  in  the  stafif  notation  are  the 
usual  tempered  scale,  but  the  inserted  duodenals 
convert  them  into  just  notes,  on  the  principle 
of  App.  XX.  sect.  £.  art.  26.  The  tonic  is 
taken  as  B,  in  order  to  be  within  the  duo^ne 
of  C,  and  hence  the  subdominanr  is  j^|  and  - 
the  dominant  i^,K  ,  giving  the  three  duodenes : 
In  Ex.  I  the  [Bj]  indicates  that  the  first  two 


Subdominant  J?| 

Tonic  By 

Dominant  F,ff 

D      F,n     A,n 

A       C,«     E^ 

E      G,t     B,5 

0      B,       DS 

D      F,l     A.,l 

A      C,«     E^ 

C      Fs,       0,ff 

0      JB,       D^t 

D      F,t     Aji 

F      A,       C,ff 

C      E,       G,n        ■ 

0      B,       Dji 

chords  are  in  the  duodene  of  B,.  Then  [E{\  shews 
that  the  Dext  two  chords  are  in  the  duodene 
of  Ey.  The  difference  relates  to  the  chords 
with  A  in  the  tirst  case  and  A^  in  the  second. 
But  the  next  pair  of  chords  return  to  the 
duodene  of  B,,  which  remains  till  the  last  bar, 
when  the  notes  are  in  the  duodene  of  F^Xi . 
This  is  rendered  necessary  by  the  chord  of  the 
Seventh  on  C,S  the  second  of  the  scale,  the 
Fifth  of  which  is  G,ff  and  not  Gf-J  ,  which  is  the 
Sixth  of  the  scale  of  /?,.  That  is,  it  is  C,ff  +  E.,l 


—  G,5  I  J5,.  This  is,  however,  only  a  tem- 
porary modulation,  and  the  piece  ends  in  the 
d u odene  of  B, .  In  E x.  2  the  modulations  are  only 
J5,.  F,5  and  B,,  that  into  F^t  being  necessitated 
by  the  same  chord  as  before.  If  these  modu- 
lations were  not  taken,  but  the  duodene  of  B, 
were  persisted  in  throughout,  frightful  dis- 
sonances (much  worse  than  the  old  *  wolves ') 
would  ensue  from  the  imperfect  Fifths  E^A 
and  C,«  GjU  .-Translator,] 


Digitized  by  VjOOQIC 


OHAF.  XT. 


HABMONISATION  OF  THE  TONAL  MODES. 


305 


2.    [Bi-]      I'M 


[Bil 


i 


Ei& 


i 


^     U  1^^"^="^^ 


V,  iTJ  -^ 


.-^  ^  ^ 


^E^ 


y 


* 


g 


There  are  many  similar  examples.    He  evidently  evades  a  regular  olose. 

Minob-Majob  Mode. 

Modem  composers,  when  they  wish  to  insert  a  tonal  mode  which  lies  between  ^ 
Major  and  Minor,  to  be  used  for  a  few  phrases  or  cadences,  have  generally  preferred 
giving  the  minor  chord  of  the  mode  to  the  subdominant  and  not  to  the  tonic. 
Hauptmann  calls  this  the  minor-major  mode  {Moll'Durtonart).*    Its  chain  of 
chords  ] 


I ^^1 I 


^his  gives  a  leading  note  in  the  dominant  chord,  and  a  complete  final  cadence  in 
the  major  chord  of  the  tonic,  while  the  minor  relation  of  the  subdominant  chord 
remains  undisturbed.  This  minor-major  mode  is  at  all  events  much  more  suitable 
for  harmonisation  than  the  old  mode  of  the  minor  Seventh.  But  it  is  unsuitable 
for  homophonic  singing,  unless  in  the  ascending  scale  a^[>  is  changed  into  ai, 
because  the  voice  would  otherwise  have  to  make  the  complicated  step  a*[>. . .  &i  [=  2 74 
cents,  see  p.  301a,  d].  The  old  modes  were  derived  from  homophonic  singing,  for  ^ 
which  the  mode  of  the  minor  Seventh  is  perfectly  well  fitted,  as  we  know  from  its 
being  still  used  as  our  ascending  minor  8cale.t 

Mode  of  the  Mikob  Sixth. 

While  the  mode  of  the  minor  Seventh  oscillates  indeterminately  between  major 
and  minor  without  admitting  of  any  consistent  treatment,  the  m^de  of  the  minor 
Sixth  (Greek  Doric,  [p.  274^',  note  No.  7]  ecclesiastical  Phrygian),  with  its  minor 
Second,  has  a  much  more  peculiar  character,  which  distinguishes  it  altogether  from 
all  other  modes.  This  minor  Second  stands  in  the  same  melodic  connection  to  the 
tonic  as  a  leading  note  would  do,  but  it  requires  a  descending  progression.  Hence 
for  descending  passages  this  mode  possesses  the  same  melodic  advantages  as  the 
major  mode  does  for  ascending  passages.  The  minor  Second  has  the  more  distant 
relationship  with  the  tonic,  due  entirely  to  the  subdominant.  The  mode  cannot  If 
form  a  dominant  chord  without  exceeding  its  limits.  If  we  keep  c  as  the  tonic, 
the  chain  of  chords  is 


In  this  case  the  chords  6(>— d^|>-|-/and  ^^[>+/— a'^)  are  not  directly  related  to  the 
tonic.  The  tone  d^\}  cannot  enter  into  any  consonant  chord  which  is  directly 
related  to  the  tonic.  But  since  d^\}  is  the  characteristic  minor  Second  of  the  mode, 
such  chords  cannot  well  be  avoided,  not  even  in  the  cadence.    Although,  then. 


*  [It  is  I  C  mijiia.ma.  of  App.  XX.  sect.  E. 
art.  g,— Translator,] 

f  [After  the  introduction  of  the  leading  note 
to  form  a  major  dominant  chord. — Translator.] 

^  [The  notes  have  been  transposed  in 
order  to  keep  the  same  tonic  chord  C—E^b  +  O. 


Obserre  that  both  Sevenths  hb  and  b^b  are  in- 
troduced. If  &'b  be  omitted,  the  system  of 
chords  is  that  of  5  ^  mi.mi.mi.  On  the  Har- 
monica!, on  account  of  the  absence  of  6 b»  it  is 
necessary  to  use  the  system  of  chords  e2,  — /+ 
a,  -  c  +  JK,  -  G  +  JBi  -  d.'-TranslatorJ} 


Digitized  by  Vj03DQl€ 


3o6  EEMNANTS  OF  OLD  TONAL  MODES.  pabtih. 

there  is  a  close  relationship  between  the  consecntive  links  of  the  chain  of  chords, 
some  of  its  indispensable  terms  are  only  distantly  related  to  the  tonic.  Moreover, 
in  the  course  of  a  piece  in  this  mode,  it  will  always  be  necessary  to  form  the 
dominant  chord  g-i-bi^d*  although  it  contains  two  tones  foreign  to  the  original 
mode,  as  otherwise  we  could  not  prevent  the  prevalence  of  the  impression  that/ 
is  the  tonic  and/— a*[>+c  the  tonic  chord.  It  follows,  therefore,  that  the  mode  of 
the  minor  Sixth  must  be  still  less  consistent  in  its  harmonisation  and  still  more 
loosely  connected  than  the  minor  mode,  although  it  admits  of  very  consistent 
melodic  treatment.  It  contains  three  essential  minor  chords,  namely  the  tonic 
c— e^b  +  ^r,  the  subdominant /—a'b+c,  and  the  chord  which  contains  the  two  tones 
slightly  related  to  the  tonic  H^—d^\}+f,  It  is  exactly  the  reverse  of  the  major 
mode,  for  whereas  that  mode  proceeds  towards  the  dominant,  this  mode  proceeds 
towards  the  subdominant. 

f     Major:  f+a^   -C+JS?i  ^G+b^-d 

Mode  of  I  I  I 

minor  Sixth :     6t>-d>b+/-a'l>+0-JS7>b+C? 

For  harmonisation  the  difference  of  the  two  cases  is,  first,  that  the  related  tones 
introduced  into  the  sgale  by  the  subdominant  /,  namely  ^  and  d^\}j  are  not 
partials  of  the  compound  tone  of  the  subdominant,  whereas  tones  hi  and  d,  which 
are  introduced  by  the  dominant,  are  some  of  the  partials  of  the  tonic ;  and, 
secondly,  that  the  tonic  chord  always  lies  on  the  dominant  side  of  the  tonic  tone. 
Hence  in  the  harmonic  connection,  the  tones  l^BXid  d^\)  cannot  be  so  closely  united 
with  either  the  tonic  tone  or  the  tonic  chord,  as  is  the  case  with  the  supplementary 
tones  introduced  by  the  dominant.  This  gives  a  kind  of  exaggerated  minor  cha- 
racter to  the  mode  of  the  minor  Sixth,  when  harmonised.  Its  tones  and  chords  are 
certainly  connected,  but  much  less  clearly  and  intelligibly  than  those  of  the  minor 

IT  system.  The  chords  which  can  be  brought  together  in  this  key,  without  obscuring 
reference  to  c  as  the  tonic,  are  ^  minor  and  d^\}  major  on  the  one  hand  and  g 
major  on  the  other,  chords  which  in  the  major  system  could  not  be  brought  together 
without  extraordinary  modulational  appUances.f  The  esthetical  character  of  the 
mode  of  the  minor  Sixth  corresponds  with  this  fact.  It  is  well  suited  for  the 
expression  of  dark  mystery,  or  of  deepest  depression,  and  an  utter  lapse  into 
melancholy,  in  which  it  is  impossible  to  collect  one's  thoughts.  On  the  other 
hand,  as  its  descending  leading  note  gives  it  a  certain  amount  of  energy  in  descent, 
it  is  able  to  express  earnest  and  majestic  solemnity,  to  which  the  concurrence  of 
those  major  chords  which  are  so  strangely  connected  gives  a  kind  of  peculiar 
magnificence  and  wondrous  richness. 

Notwithstanding  that  the  mode  of  the  minor  Sixth  has  been  rejected  from 
modem  musical  theory,  much  more  distinct  traces  of  its  existence  have  been  left 
in  musical  practice  than  of  any  other  ancient  mode  ;  for  the  mode  of  the  Fourth 

IT  has  been  fiised  into  the  major,  and  the  mode  of  the  minor  Seventh  into  the  minor. 
Certainly  a  mode  like  that  we  have  described  is  not  suitable  for  frequent  use ;  it  is 
not  closely  enough  connected  for  long  pieces,  but  its  peculiar  power  of  expression 
cannot  be  replaced  by  that  of  any  other  mode.  Its  occurrence  is  generally  marked 
by  its  peculiar  final  cadence  which  starts  from  the  minor  Second  in  the  root.  In 
Handel  the  natural  cadence  of  this  system  is  used  with  great  effect.    Thus  in  the 

*  [The  introdaction  of  this  chord  shews  3  il'b  majna.ma.  of  App.XX.  sect.  E.  art.  9. — 

that  the  composer  is  writing  in  the  key  of  c,  but  Translator.] 

has  a  prevailing  tendency  to  modulate  into  the  t  [This,  in  fact,  lengthens  the  original  chain 

subdominant,  from  which  6 b ,  <i' b  are  chosen,  of  chords  into  bb-d}b  +f—a^b +c—e^b -h g  + 

When  &'b  is  used  for  6b,  or  b^  for  &*b,  the  5, -<2,  and  leads  to  the  treatment  of  the  mode 

modulation  into  the  subdominant   does  not  as  merely  C  minor,  with  a  tendency  to  modu- 

take  place.    The  major  chord  e*b  +  ^— 6'b  is  late  into  F  minor.    The  C  minor  is,  however, 

entirely  adventitious.    If  it  is  used  in  ascend-  the  modem  minor  C  mi.mi.ma.,  and  the  F 

ing,  thus,  c  112  d'b  204  ^'b  182/204^  112  minor  is  F  mi.mi.mi.,  whidi  is  much  more 

a*b  204  &*b  182  c\  the  result  is  the  scale  of  gloomy, ^Translator.] 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


CHAP.  XV. 


EEMNANTS  OF  OLD  TONAL  MODES. 


307 


Messiah,  the  magnificent  fagae  And  with  his  stripes  we  are  healed,  which  has  the 
signature  of  F  minor,  but  by  its  frequent  use  of  the  harmony  of  the  dominant 
Seventh  on  G,  shews  that  C  is  the  real  tonic,  introduces  the  pure  [ecclesiastical 
Phrygian]  Doric  cadence  as  follows  :  • 


[^0 


^ 


^ 


^ 


521 


lar 


132= 


122: 


=22= 


^ 


^ 


i 


J  J. 


m 


zjan 


-7zr 


Similarly  in  /SarrMon,t  the  chorus,  flie^r,  Jacob's  God,  which,  written  in  the  ^ 
Doric  mode  of  E,  finely  characterises  the  earnest  prayer  of  the  anxious  Israelites 
as  contrasted  with  the  noisy  sacrificial  songs  of  the  Philistines  in  G  major,  which 
immediately  follow.    The  cadence  here  also  is  purely  Doric.it 


The  chorus  of  Israelites  which  introduces  the  third  pwrt :  In  Thwnder  come,  0  God,  ^ 
from  heaven  I  and  is  chiefly  in  A  minor,  has  likely  an  intermediate  Doric  section. 

Sebastian  Bach  also,  in  the  chorales  which  he  has  harmonised,  has  left  them 
in  the  mode  of  the  minor  Sixth,  to  which  they  melodically  belonged,  whenever  the 
text  requires  a  deeply  sorrowful  expression,  as  in  the  De  Proftmdis  or  the  AtLS 
tiefer  Noth  schreV  ich  zu  dir,  and  again  in  Paul  Gerhardt's  song,  Wenn  ich  einmal 
soil  scheiden,  so  scheide  nicht  von  mvr.  But  he  has  harmonised  the  same  melody 
arranged  for  other  texts,  as  Befiehl  Du  deine  Wege,  and  0  Haupt  von  Blut  und 
Wunden,  &c.,  as  major  or  minor,  in  which  case  the  melody  ends  on  the  Third  or 
Fifth  of  the  key,  instead  of  on  the  Doric  tonic. 

Fortlage  §  had  already  observed  that  Mozart  had  applied  the  Doric  mode  in 


*  [The  cadence  is  produced  by  passing 
from  the  minor  subdominani  Bb^D^b  +  F  to 
the  major  dominant,  C  +  E^  —  0,m  the  key  of 
F  minor.  This  is  the  concluding  cadence  of 
the  whole  fugne,  and  for  this  reason  appa- 
rently, the  signature  in  Novello's  edition  is 
that  of  C  minor,  not  of  F  minor,  and  the  d^  b 
18  n&arked  as  an  accidental  throughout.  That 
is,  Kovello  takes  the  key  to  be  C  minor  with  a 
constant  tendency  to  modulate  into  the  key  of 
the  subdominant,  from  which  it  borrows  the 
chord  Bb—D^b  +  F,  But  the  fugue  begins 
with  F...f  in  the  bass,  and  the  opening  sub- 
ject, in  the  treble,is  c",  a"b,  d»"b, «/,  /,  g",  a»'b, 
6'b,  c",  which  is  clearly  in  the  scale  of  F  minor, 
with  the  chordal  system  bb—d^b  +f-a^  b  +  c  +  e, 
— ^,  of  which  it  contains  every  note.  In  the 
text  ihe  [FJ]  is  the  duodenal  and  refers  to 
the  duodene  of  F,  which  contains  all  the  tones 
in  the  passage.  The  whole  fugue  oscillates 
between  the  duodenes  C  and  F.— Translator.] 

f  [Mr.  H.  Keatley  Moore  informs  me  that 


this  chorus  was  taken  by  Handel  from 
PloratefiMae  Israel  in  Carisaimi's  Jephthah, 
— Translator.] 

X  [The  duodene  is  that  of  A^.  The  sncces-  m 
sion  of  chords,  each  reduced  to  the  simplest 
form,  as  referred  to  by  the  bracketed  figures 
below  the  notes,  is  i.  e,— ^  +  6„  2.  a^  —  c  +  e^, 
3.  e^-g  +  b^,  4.  Z+Oi-c,  S-<^i-/+«ii  6.  e,+ 
i7«5  —  6,  7-  «!— c  +  e„  8.  e^+g^—bi.  Hence, 
assuming  the  scale  to  have  the  chordal  system 
d,-/+a,-c  +  e,-5f  +  6„  with  e,-gf  +  6,  as  the 
tonic  chord,  taken  major  as e^+  g^  -^biin  the 
close,  we  have  the  *  Boric  cadence'  between 
chords  5  and  6,  which  is  then  lengthened  by 
introducing  the  remaining  tones  of  the  key  in 
7,  the  whole  closing  as  in  8.  It  would  be  most 
probably  received  as  in  A^  minor,  closing  in 
the  dominant. — Translator.] 

§  Examples  from  instrumental  music  are 
mentioned  by  Ekert  in  his  Habilitationsschrift, 
Die  Principien  der  Modulation  und  musikal- 
ischen  Idee.    Heidelberg,  i860,  p.  12.  ^ 

Digitized  by  V^j£M3QIC 


3o8  REMNANTS  OF  OLD  TONAL  MODES.  i»abt  m. 

Famina*B  air  in  the  second  act  Of  II  Fla/uto  Magioo  [No.  19].  One  of  the  finest 
examples  for  the  contrast  between  this  and  the  major  mode  occurs  in  the  same 
composer*s  Don  Giovanni  in  the  Sestette  of  the  second  act  [No.  21],  where  Ottavio 
and  Donna  Anna  enter.    Ottavio  sings  the  comforting  words— 

Tergi  il  oiglio,  0  vita  mia, 
£  d&  calma  al  tuo  dolore 

in  D  major,  which,  however,  is  pectdiarly  coloured  by  a  preponderating,  although 
not  uninterrupted,  inclination  to  the  subdominant,  as  in  the  mode  of  the  Fourth. 
Then  Anna,  who  is  plunged  in  grief,  begins  in  perfectly  similar  melodical  phrases, 
and  vnth  a  similar  accompaniment,  and  after  a  short  modulation  through  D  minor, 
establishes  herself  in  the  mode  of  the  minor  Sixth  for  C,  vnth  the  words — 

Sol  la  morte,  0  mio  tesoroi 
11  n  mio  pianto  pa6  finir. 

The  contrast  between  gentle  emotion  and  crushing  grief  is  here  represented  with  a 
most  wonderfully  beautiful  effect,  principally  by  the  change  of  mode.  The  dying 
Commandant  also,  in  the  introduction  to  Don  Giovanni,  ends  with  a  Doric  cadence.* 
Similarly  the  Agni^  Dei  of  Mozart's  Bequiem-^aliiioxxgh,  of  course,  we  (ure  not 
quite  certain  how  much  of  this  was  vmtten  by  himself. 

Among  Beethoven's  compositions  we  may  notice  the  first  movement  of  the 
Sonata,  No.  90,  in  E  minor,  for  the  pianoforte,  as  an  example  of  peculiar  de- 
pression caused  by  repeated  Doric  cadences,  whence  the  second  (major)  movement 
acquires  a  still  softer  expression. 

Modem  composers  form  a  cadence  which  belongs  to  the  mode  of  the  minor 
Sixth,  by  means  of  the  minor  Second  and  the  major  Seventh,  the  so-called  chord 
of  the  extreme  sharp  Sixth,t  /'+a...e2ijjl)  where  both  />  and  dijj^  have  to  move 
f  half  a  tone  to  reach  the  tonic  e  [p.  2S6b],  This  chord  cannot  be  deduced  fix>m  the 
major  and  minor  modes,  and  hence  appears  very  enigmatical  and  inexplicable  to 
many  modem  theoreticians.  But  it  is  easily  explained  as  a  remnant  of  the  old 
mode  of  the  minor  Sixth,  in  which  the  major  Seventh  dijj^,  which  belongs  to  the 
dominant  chord  h+d^jj^-fj/^,  is  combined  vdth  the  tones  f^+a,  which  are  taken 
from  the  subdominant  side.it 

These  examples  may  suffice  to  shew  that  there  are  still  remnants  of  the  mode 
of  the  minor  Sixth  in  modem  music.  It  would  be  easy  to  adduce  more  examples 
if  they  were  looked  for.  The  harmonic  connection  of  the  chords  in  this  mode  is 
not  sufficiently  firm  and  intelligible  for  the  construction  of  long  pieces.  But  in 
short  pieces,  chorales,  or  intermediate  sections,  and  melodic  phrases  in  larger 
musical  works,  it  is  so  effective  in  its  expression,  that  it  should  not  be  forgotten, 
especially  as  Handel,  Bach,  and  Mozart  have  used  it  in  such  conspicuous  places 
in  their  works.  § 

f       *  [No.  I  of  the  opera.    Bepresenting  major  Leipzig,  1866),  has  carried  oat»  in  a  moat  in- 

chords  by  capitals  and  minor  by  small  letters,  teresting  manner,  the  complete  analogy  be- 

the  final  chords  of  the  vocal  music  are  /,  D*  bi  tween  the  mode  of  the  minor  Sixth  and  the 

0*b,  /,  C,  ft  so  that  all  the  tones  will  lie  in  major  mode,  of  which  it  is  the  direct  conTer- 

the  scheme  g*b  +  6b  -d> b  +  /  -a» b  +  c  +  «,  -  (7,  sion ;  and  has  shewn  how  this  conversion  l^da 

or  c-  e*  b  +  ^.    The  tonic  is  F. — Translator.]  to  a  peculiarly  characteristic  harmonisation  of 

f  [Callcott  {Mtuical  Qrammar,  1809,  art.  the  mode  of  the  minor  Sixth.    In  this  respect 

441)  calls  it  *the  chord  of  the  extreme  sharp  I  wish  emphatically  to  recommend  this  book 

Sixth,*  and  says  that  *  this  harmony  when  to  the  attention  of  musicians.    On  the  other 

accompanied  simply  by  the  Third,  has  been  hand,  it  seems  to  me  that  it  is  necessary  to 

termed  the  Italian  Sixth,*    Of  course  he  has  shew  by  musical  practice,  that  the  new  prin- 

no  theory  for  it ;  the  tone  is  *  accidentally  ciple,  which  is  made  the  basis  of  that  writer's 

sharpened.' — Translator.]  theory  of  the  mode  of  the  minor  Sixth,  con- 

i  [That  is   the  chords  of  the  scale  are  sidered  by  birn   as  the  theoretically  normal 

taken  as  (2  -/*  +  a  -  c*  +  e  -  g*  +  6  +  (iff  — ^ ,  of  minor  mode,  really  suffices  for  the  construction 

which  the  two  notes  last  are  modem  additions.'  of  great  musical  pieces.    The  author,  namely. 

See  p.  2S6d,  note  f.— Translator.]  considers  the  minor  triad  c— e'b  +  g  as  repre- 

§  Herr  A.  von  Oettingcn,  in  his  Harmonie-  senting  the  tone  g"  which  is  common  to  the 

system  in  dualer  Entwickelujtg  (Dorpat  and  three  compound  tones  of  which  it  is  composed 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


CHAP.  XV. 


TEICHORDAL  TONAL  MODES- 


309 


Similar  relations  exist  for  the  mode  of  the  Fourth  and  of  the  minor  Seventh, 
although  these  are  less  specifically  different  from  the  major  and  minor  modes  re- 
spectively.  They  are,  however,  capable  of  giving  a  peculiar  expression  to  certain 
musical  periods,  although  dif^culties  would  arise  in  consistently  carrying  out  these 
peculiarities  through  long  pieces  of  music.  The  harmonic  phrases  which  belong  to 
these  two  last-named  modes  can  of  course  also  be  executed  within  the  usual  major 
and  minor  systems.  But  perhaps  it  would  fEM^ilitate  the  theoretical  comprehen- 
sion of  certain  modulations,  if  the  conception  of  these  modes  and  of  their  system 
of  harmonisation  were  definitely  laid  down. 

The  only  point,  then,  as  historical  development  and  physiological  theory  alike 
testify,  for  which  modem  music  is  superior  to  the  ancient,  is  harmonisation.  The 
development  of  modem  music  has  been  evoked  by  its  theoretical  principle,  that  the 
tonic  chord  should  predominate  among  the  series  of  chords  by  the  same  laws  of 
relationship  as  the  tonic  note  predominates  among  the  notes  of  the  scale.  This  If 
principle  did  not  become  practically  efiective  till  the  commencement  of  last  century, 
when  it  was  felt  necessary  to  preserve  the  minor  chord  in  the  final  cadence. 

The  physiological  phenomenon  which  this  esthetical  principle  brought  into 
action,  is  the  compound  character  of  musical  tones  which  are  of  themselves  chords 
composed  of  partials,  and  consequently,  conversely,  the  possibility  under  certain 
circumstances  of  replacing  compound  tones  by  chords.  Hence  in  every  chord  the 
principal  tone  is  that  of  which  the  whole  chord  may  be  considered  to  express  its 
compound  form.  Practically  this  principle  was  acknowledged  from  the  time  that 
pieces  of  music  were  allowed  to  end  in  chords  of  several  parts.  Then  it  was  im- 
mediately felt  that  the  concluding  tone  of  the  bass  ought  be  accompanied  by  a 
higher  Octave,  Fifth,  and,  finally,  major  Third,  but  not  by  a  Fourth,  or  minor 
Sixth,  and  for  a  long  time  also  the  minor  Third  was  rejected ;  and  we  know  that 
the  first  three  intervals  (the  Octave,  Fifth,  and  major  Third)  occur  among  the 
partials  of  the  compound  tone  which  lies  in  the  bass,  and  that  the  others  do  not.    f 

The  various  values  of  the  tones  of  a  chord  were  first  theoretically  recognised 
by  Bameau  in  his  theory  of  the  fundamental  bass,  although  Bameau  was  not  ac- 
quainted with  the  cause  here  assigned  for  these  different  values.  That  compound 
tone  which  represents  a  chord  according  to  our  view,  constitutes  its  Fundamental 
Bas8^  Radical  Tone  or  Boot^  as  distinguished  from  its  hass^  that  is,  the  tone  which 
belongs  to  the  lowest  part.  The  major  triad  has  the  same  TOot  whatever  be  its 
inversion  or  position.  In  the  chords  c+ 6]—^,  orgr...c+ei,therootisstillc.  The 
minor  chord  d^P-\-a  has  also  as  a  rule  only  d  as  its  root  in  all  its  inversions,  but 
in  the  chord  of  ttie  great  [or  added]  Sixth/*  +  a... (i^  we  may  also  consider/*  as  the 
root,  and  it  is  in  this  sense  that  it  occurs  in  the  cadence  of  c*  major.  Bameau's 
successors  have  partly  given  up  this  last  distinction ;  but  it  is  one  in  which  Bameau's 
fine  artistic  feeling  fully  corresponded  with  the  facts  in  nature.  The  minor  chord 
really  admits  of  this  double  interpretation,  as  we  have  already  shewn  (p.  294^). 

The  essential  difference  between  the  old  and  new  tonal  modes  is  this :  the  old  ^ 
have  their  minor  chords  on  the  dominant,  the  new  on  the  subdominant  side. 

The  reasons  for  the  following  construction  have  been  already  investigated.* 


In  the 

The  chord  of  the 

Subdominant 

l8 

Tonio 

iB 

■  •      ■■-> 

Dominant 

is 

( Mode  of  the  minor  Third  . 
Old-i  Mode  of  the  minor  Seventh 
(Mode  of  the  Fourth  . 
Major  Mode       .        .        .        . 
v^«  i  Minor-Major  Mode     . 
^^^iMinorMode       .        .        .        . 

Minor 
Major 
Major 
Major 
Minor 
Minor 

Minor 
Minor 
Major 
Major 
Major 
Minor 

Minor 
Minov 
Minor 
Majpr 
Major 
Major 

(being  a  higher  Octave  of  g,  of  the  Fifth  of  c, 
and  of  the  major  Third  of  6'b),  and  hence 
caUe  it  *  the  phonic  g  tone,*  whereas  he  con- 


siders c  +  e,— <7  in  the  same  way  as  we  do,  as 
the  *  tonic  c  tone.' 

*  [It  will  be  seen  that  this  arrangement 

Digitized  by  V^OOQ IC 


310  DIFFERENT  CHAEACTERS  OP  KEYS.  paet  m* 

CHAPTER  XVL 

THE  SYSTEM  OF  EETS. 

Thebb  is  nothing  in  the  nature  of  music  itself  to  determine  the  pitch  of  the  ionic 
of  any  composition.  If  different  melodies  and  musical  pieces  have  to  be  execated 
by  musical  instruments  or  singing  voices  of  definite  compass,  the  tonic  must  be 
chosen  of  a  suitable  pitch,  differing  when  the  melody  rises  fax  above  the  tonic  and 
when  it  sinks  much  below  it.  In  short,  the  pitch  of  the  tonic  must  be  chosen  so 
as  to  bring  the  compass  of  the  tones  of  the  piece  within  the  compass  of  the  execu- 
tants, vocal  or  instrumental.  This  inevitable  practical  necessity  entails  the  con- 
dition of  being  able  to  give  any  required  pitch  to  the  tonic. 

f  Moreover,  in  the  longer  pieces  of  music  it  is  necessary  to  be  able  to  make  a 
temporary  change  of  tonic,  that  is,  to  modulate^  in  order  to  avoid  uniformity  and 
to  utilise  the  musical  effects  resulting  &om  changing  and  then  returning  to  the 
original  key.  Just  as  consonances  are  made  more  prominent  and  effective  by  means 
of  dissonances,  the  feeling  for  the  predominant  tonality  and  the  satisfaction  which 
arises  from  it,  is  heightened  by  previous  deviations  into  adjacent  keys.  The  variety 
in  musical  turns  produced  by  modulational  connection  has  become  all  the  more 
necessary  for  modem  music,  because  we  have  been  obliged  entirely  to  renounce, 
or  at  any  rate  materially  to  circumscribe,  the  old  principle  of  altering  expression 
by  means  of  the  various  tonal  modes.  The  Greeks  had  a  free  choice  among  seven 
different  tonal  modes,  the  middle  ages  among  five  or  six,  but  we  can  choose  between 
two  only,  major  and  minor.  Those  old  tonal  modes  presented  a  series  of  different 
degrees  of  tonal  character,  out  of  which  two  only  remain  suitable  for  harmonic 
music.    But  the  clearer  and  firmer  construction  of  an  harmonic  piece  gives  modem 

f  composers  greater  freedom  in  modulational  deviations  from  the  original  key,  and 
places  at  their  command  new  sources  of  musical  wealth,  which  were  scarcely 
accessible  to  the  ancients. 

Finally  I  must  just  touch  on  the  question,  so  much  discussed,  whether  each 
different  key  has  a  peculiar  character  of  its  own. 

It  is  quite  clear  that,  within  the  course  of  a  single  piece  of  music,  modulational 
deviations  iato  the  more  or  less  distantly  related  keys  on  the  dominant  or  sub- 
dominant  side  produce  very  different  effects.  This,  however,  arises  simply  from 
the  contrast  they  offer  to  the  original  principal  key,  and  would  be  merely  a  rela- 
tive character.  But  the  question  here  mooted  is,  whether  individual  keys  have  an 
absolute  character  of  their  own,  independently  of  their  relation  to  any  other  key. 

This  is  often  asserted,  but  it  is  dif&cult  to  determine  how  much  tmth  the 
assertion  contains,  or  even  what  it  precisely  means,  because  probably  a  variety  of 
different  things  are  included  under  the  term  character^  and  perhaps  the  amount  of 

H  effect  due  to  the  particular  instrument  employed  has  not  been  allowed  for.  If  an 
instrument  of  fixed  tones  is  completely  and  uniformly  tuned  according  to  the  equal 
temperament,  so  that  all  Semitones  throughout  the  scale  have  precisely  the  same 
magnitude,  and  if  also  the  musical  quality  of  all  the  tones  is  precisely  the  same, 
there  seems  to  be  no  ground  for  understanding  how  each  different  key  should  have 
a  different  character.  Musicians  folly  capable  of  forming  a  judgment  have  also 
admitted  to  me,  that  no  difference  in  the  character  of  the  keys  can  be  observed  on 
the  organ,  for  example.  And  Hauptmann,*  I  think,  is  right  when  he  makes  the 
same  assertion  for  singing  voices  with  or  without  an  organ  accompaniment.  A 
great  change  in  the  pitch  of  the  tonic  can  at  most  cause  all  the  higher  notes  to  be 
strained  or  all  the  lower  ones  obscured. 

On  the  other  hand,  there  is  a  decidedly  different  character  in  different  keys  on 

does  not  inolnde  the  mode  of  the  minor  Sixth,      art.  9,  and  thence  to  the  general  theory  of 
It  was  this  tabulation  which  led  me  to  the      duodenes  in  that  section. — TVansZafor.] 
richordal  theory  developed  in  App.  XX.  sect.  E.  *  Harmonik  und  Metrik,  p.  188. 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


CHAP.  XVI.  DIFFERENT  CHARACTEEB  OF  KEYS.  311 

pianofortes  and  bowed  instmments.  0  major  and  the  adjacent  D\>  major  have 
different  effects.  That  this  difference  is  not  caused  by  difference  of  absolute  pitch, 
can  be  readily  determined  by  comparing  two  different  instruments  tuned  to  differ- 
ent pitches.  The  D\}  of  the  one  instrument  may  be  as  high  as  the  G  of  the  other, 
and  yet  on  both  the  G  major  retains  its  brighter  and  stronger  character,  and  the 
X>t>  its  soft  and  veiled  harmonious  effect.  It  is  scarcely  possible  to  think  of  any 
oth^  reason  than  that  the  method  of  striking  the  short  narrow  black  digitals  of 
the  piano  must  produce  a  somewhat  different  quality  of  tone,  and  that  difference  of 
character  arises  &om  the  different  distribution  of  the  stronger  and  gentler  quality 
of  tone  among  the  different  degrees  of  the  scale.*  The  difference  made  in  the 
timing  of  those  Fifths  which  the  tuner  keeps  to  the  last,  and  on  which  are  crowded 
the  whole  of  the  errors  in  tuning  the  other  Fifths  in  the  circle  of  Fifths,  may 
possibly  be  regular,  and  may  contribute  to  this  effect,  but  of  this  I  have  no  person^ 
experience.     [See  App.  XX.  sect.  G.  art.  17.]  f 

In  bowed  instruments  the  more  powerful  quality  of  tone  in  the  open  strings  is 
conspicuous,  and  there  are  also  probably  differences  in  the  quality  of  tone  of  strings 
which  are  stopped  at  short  and  long  lengths,  and  these  may  alter  the  character  of 
the  key  according  to  the  degree  of  the  scale  on  which  they  fall.  This  assumption 
is  confirmed  by  the  inquiries  I  have  made  of  musicians  respecting  the  mode  in 
which  they  recognise  keys  under  certain  conditions.  The  inequaHty  of  intonation 
wiQ  add  to  this  effect.  The  Fifths  of  the  open  strings  are  perfect  Fifths.  But  it 
is  impossible  that  all  the  other  Fifths  should  be  perfect  if  in  playing  in  different 
keys  e%ch  note  has  the  same  sound  throughout,  as  appears  at  least  to  be  the  inten- 
tion of  elementary  instruction  on  the  violin.  In  this  way  the  scales  of  the  various 
keys  will  differ  in  intonation,  and  this  will  necessarily  have  a  much  more  important 
influence  on  the  character  of  the  melody.     [See  App.  XX.  sect.  O.  arts.  6  and  7.] 

The  differences  in  quality  of  tone  of  different  notes  on  wind  instruments  are 
still  more  striking.  ^ 

If  this  view  is  correct,  the  character  of  the  keys  would  be  very  different  on  differ- 
ent instruments,  and  I  believe  this  to  be  the  case.  But  it  is  a  matter  to  be  decided 
by  a  musician  with  delicate  ears,  who  directs  his  attention  to  the  points  here  raised. 

It  is,  however,  not  impossible  that  by  a  peculiarity  of  the  human  ear,  abeady 
touched  upon  in  p.  11 6a,  certain  common  features  may  enter  into  the  character  of 
keys,  independent  of  the  difference  of  musical  instruments,  and  dependent  solely 
on  the  absolute  pitch  of  the  tonic.  Since  ^'"'  is  a  proper  tone  of  the  human  ear, 
it  sounds  peculiarly  shrill  under  ordinary  circumstances,  and  somewhat  of  this 
shrillness  is  common  to /'"J  and  a""l>.  To  a  somewhat  less  extent  those  musical 
tones  of  which  ^""  is  an  upper  partial,  as  gr"',  0"',  and  gf",  have  a  brighter  and  more 
piercing  tone  than  their  neighbours.  It  is  possible,  then,  that  it  is  not  indifferent 
for  pieces  in  G  major  to  have  its  high  Fifth  g'^  and  high  tonic  c'"  thus  distinguished 
in  brightness  from  other  tones,  but  these  differences  must  in  all  cases  be  very  slight, 
and  for  the  present  I  must  leave  it  undecided  whether  they  have  any  weight  at  all.  ^ 

All  or  some  of  these  reasons,  then,  made  it  necessary  for  musicians  to  have  free 
command  over  the  pitch  of  the  tonic,  and  hence  even  the  later  Greeks  transposed 
their  scales  on  to  all  degrees  of  the  chromatic  scale.  For  singers  these  trans- 
positions offer  no  difficulties.  They  can  begin  with  any  required  pitch,  and  find 
in  their  vocal  instrument  all  such  of  the  corresponding  degrees  as  lie  within  the 
extreme  limits  of  their  voice.  But  the  matter  becomes  much  more  difficult  for 
musical  instruments,  especially  for  such  as  only  possess  tones  of  certain  definite 
degrees  of  pitch.  The  difficulty  is  not  entirely  removed  even  on  bowed  instruments. 
It  is  true  that  these  can  produce  every  required  degree  of  pitch  ;  but  players  are 
unable  to  hit  the  pitch,  as  correctly  as  the  ear  desires,  without  acquiring  a  certain 

*  [Mr.  H.  Eeatley  Moore,  Mas.  B.,  thinks  they  gain  this  by  a  quicker  motion,  each  arm 

the  difference  is  due  to  the  different  leverage  of  the  lever   being  shorter,   and  short  keys 

of  the  black  digitals.    Although  in  well  con-  differing  altogether  from  long  ones  in  the  feel- 

structed    digitals  the    black   have  as  much  ing  produced  in  the  hand.    See  also  App.  XX. 

action  at  the  further  end  as  the  white  ones,  sect.  N.  No.  6. — Translator.] 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQ IC 


3X2  TEMPEBED  INTONATION.  .pabt  m. 

mechanical  use  of  their  fingers,  which  can  only  result  from  an  immense  amoant 
of  practice. 

The  Greek  system  was  not  accompanied  with  great  difficulties,  even  for  injstra- 
ments,  so  long  as  no  deviations  into  remote  keys  were  permitted,  and  hence  but 
few  marks  of  sharps  and  flats  had  to  be  used.  Up  to  the  beginning  of  the  seven- 
teenth century  musicians  were  content  with  two  signs  of  depression  for  the  notes 
B\}  and  E\},  and  with  the  sign  $  for  ^,  df^^  O^,  in  order  to  have  the  leading 
tones  for  the  tonics  G,  D,  and  A.  They  took  care  to  avoid  the  enharmonicallj 
equivalent  tones  il  J  for  B^},  Djj^  for  E\},  0\}  for  i^,  D\>  for  C#,  and  A\}  for  G^. 
By  help  of  B\}  for  B*  every  tonal  mode  could  be  tremsposed  to  the  key  of  its  sab- 
dominant,  and  no  other  transposition  was  made. 

In  the  Pythagorean  system,  which  maintained  its  predominance  over  theory 
to  the  time  of  Zarlino  in  the  sixteenth  century,  tuning  proceeded  by  ascending 
IF  Fifths,  thus— 

CaDAEBFJj^C^GiDjj^Ajj^EJiBH^ 
Now  if  we  tune  two  Fifths  upwards  and  an  Octave  downwards,  we  make  a  step 
having  the  ratio  f  x  f  x  ^=f ,  which  is  a  major  Second.    This  gives  for  the  pitch  of 
every  second  tone  in  the  last  list — 

G    D    E    1%    a^    A^    B^ 
I     I    (I)'  (I)'  (ir    («)*  (f)' 
Now  if  we  proceed  c2(m^wards  by  Fifijis  from  C  we  obtain  the  series — 

G    F    B\>    E\}    A\>    D\>    G\>    C\>    iT>    B\}\>    E\>\>    A\}\}    2)t>t>. 
If  we  descend  two  Fifths  and  rise  an  Octave,  we  may  obtain  the  tones — 
0    B\}    A\}    G\}    F\}    E\}\}    D\}\} 

I  I  (1)^  m'  HY  {ir   (i)« 

If  Now  the  interval  If  )«=Mmt=i  x  UUH 

or,  approximately  (|)^=i  x  ^ 

Hence  the  tone  Bjj^  is  higher  than  the  Octave  of  G  by  the  small  interval 
^  [=24  cents] ,  and  the  tone  D[}\}  is  lower  than  the  Octave  below  G  by  the  same 
interval.  If  we  ascend  by  perfect  Fifths  from  G  and  i>t>t>,  we  shall  find  the  same 
constant  difference  between 

G        a        D       A        E      B      1%    G%    Gi    D%    Ai    Ejf^  Bjtfiad 
D\}\}    A\}\>    E\^\}    B\}\>    F^    G\}    G\}    D\}    A\}    E\>    B\^    F    G. 

The  tones  in  the  upper  line  are  all  higher  than  those  in  the  lower  by  the  small 
interval  ||  [=24  cents].  Our  staff  nptation  had  its  principles  settled  before  the 
development  of  the  modem  musical  system,  and  has  consequently  preserved  these 
differences  of  pitch.  But  for  practice  on  instruments  with  fixed  tones  the  distinc- 
5)  tion  between  degrees  of  tone  which  lie  so  near  to  each  other,  was  inconvenient,  and 
attempts  were  made  to  fiise  them  together.  This  led  to  many  imperfect  attempts, 
in  which  individual  intervals  were  more  or  less  altered  in  order  to  keep  the  rest 

*  [In  the  oldest  printed  book  on  mnsic,  almost  the  cursive  ^written  form  f}   of  Ii   in 

{Franchini   Oafori   Laudenaia   Muaici  pro-  Germany.    Qn  the  other  hand  it  was  often  . 

fessorU  theoricum  opus  artnonice  discipline,  made  with  two  strokes  ||  afterwards  crossed, 

Neapolis  M.CCCC.LXXX.,  for  a  sight  of  which  like  B  ,  and  then  it  degenerated  into  ||  »  which 

I  am  indebted  to  Mr.  Quaritch,  who  bought  it  is  apparently  the  precursor  of  our  8  .    In  tiiis 

at  the  sale  of  the  SysUm  Library)  6  b  is  in  the  case  both  q   and  S   and  also  h  would  have 

printed  text  represented  by  a  small  Boman  arisen  from  the  same  square-bottomed  b»  the 

b,  and  &l|  by  a  capital  Boman  B.    But  in  a  French  bicarrey  and  PraBtorius  Q  quadratum^ 

plate  attached  are  given  eight  varieties  of  the  which,  however,  he  identified  witii  h,  H  in 

written  form  of  &Q ,  by  which  it  would  seem  subsequent  writing.     The  Italian  names  for 

to  have  been  intended  for  b   with  a  square  h,b\>9ktQsiminoretS%maggiore,  Whether  these 

instead  of  a  round  bottom,  like  0  ,  which  is  refer  to  the  musical  intervals  a  5bi  a  6l| ,  which 

almost    indistinguishable   from    a   mutilated  Gafori  printed  a  b,  a  B,  or  to  these  printed 

Boman   b.    As  it  was  clearly  made  in  two  forms,  it  is  difficult  to  say  with  certainty, 

parts  t  1 ,  the  second  was  often  long,  and  then  The  Germans  accepted  the  forms  b  b ,  as  b  h, 

the  resemblance  to  Q  was  great,  and  this  was  calling  the    latter   ha.     The   meaning    that 

Digitized  by  V^OOQIC 


CHAP.  ZVI. 


TEMPEEED  INTONATION. 


313 


true,  producing  the  so-oalled  unequal  temperaments,  and  finally  to  the  system 
of  equal  temperament,  in  which  the  Octave  was  divided  into  12  precisely  equal 
degrees  of  tone.*  We  have  seen  that  we  can  ascend  from  C  by  12  perfect  Fifths  to 
B$,  which  differs  firom  c  by  about  j^  of  a  Semitone,  namely  by  the  interval  |}.  In 
the  same  way  we  can  descend  by  12  perfect  Fifths  to  D\)>\},  which  is  as  much  lower 
than  C,  as  ^  is  higher.  If,  then,  we  put  G^BJj^=iD\}\},  and  distribute  this  httle 
deviation  of  J4  equally  among  all  the  12  Fifths  of  the  circle,  each  Fifth  will  be 
erroneous  by  about  |^  of  a  Semitone  [or  ^  of  a  comma  or  2  cents],  which  is 
certainly  a  very  small  interval.  By  this  means  all  varieties  of  tonal  degrees  within 
an  Octave  are  reduced  to  12,  as  on  our  modem  keyed  instruments. 

The  Fifth  in  the  system  of  equal  temperament,  is,  when  expressed  approximately 
in  the  smallest  possible  numbers,ss  f  x  |f  f .  It  is  very  seldom  that  any  difficulty 
could  result  from  its  use  in  place  of  the  perfect  Fifth.  The  root  struck  with  its 
tempered  Fifth  makes  one  beat  in  the  time  that  the  Fifth  makes  442^  complete  If 
vibrations.  Mow  since  a'  makes  440  vibrations  in  a  second,  it  follows  that  the 
tempered  Fifth  d'dta'  will  produce  exactly  one  beat  in  a  second.  In  long-sustained 
tones  this  would,  indeed,  be  perceptible,  but  by  no  means  disturbing,  and  for 
quick  passages  it  would  have  no  time  to  occur.  The  beats  are  still  less  disturb- 
ing in  lower  positions,  where  they  decrease  in  rapidity  with  the  pitch  numbers  of 
the  tones.  In  higher  positions  they  certainly  become  more  striking ;  d'"±^o}'* 
gives  four,  and  a'"±.e"'  six  beats  in  a  second ;  but  chords  very  seldom  occur  with 
such  high  notes  in  slow  passages.  The  Fourths  of  the  equal  temperament  are 
$  X  iff  [=49^  +  2  cents].  There  is  one  beat  for  every  22i|  vibrations  of  the  lower 
tone  of  the  Fourth.  Hence  the  Fourth  a  .  d'  makes  one  beat  in  a  second,  the 
same  as  the  Fifth  d'±:a'.  The  pure  consonances  retained  in  the  Pythagorean 
system  are  therefore  not  injured  to  any  extent  worth  notice  by  equal  temperament. 
In  melodic  progression  of  tones  the  interval  |||  borders  on  the  very  limits  of  dis- 
tinguishable differences  of  pitch,  according  to  Preyer's  experiments  (see  p.  147&).  H 
In  the  doubly  accented  Octave  it  would  be  easily  distinguished.  In  the  unac- 
cented or  lower  Octaves  it  would  not  be  felt  at  all. 

The  Thirds  and  Sixths  of  the  equal  temperament  are  nearer  the  perfect 
intervals  than  are  the  Pythagorean.f 


Intervals 

Perfect 

Equally  Tempered 

Major  Third    • 

ntioa  cents 

1     386 

ratioe        cents 

fxta    400 

xatios         cents 

Sxij    408 

Minor  Sixth    • 

!      814 

i^m    800 

fxK       79» 

Biinor  Third   . 

1       3>6 

JxiH      300 

fxH        a94 

Major  Sixth    . 

i       884 

fxifi         900 

IxfJ        906 

Semitone 

if        '8a 

BorJfHi        100 

IHorHxJf         90 

The  dissonances  occasioned  by  the  upper  partial  tones  are  consequently  some-  IF 
what  milder  than  those  due  to  Pythagorean  intervals,  but  the  combinational  tones 


Oafori  attached  to  b  B  (which  in  one  plate  he 
also  gives  in  the  same  sense  in  black  letter), 
is  shewn  by  the  following  quotation  which  he 
makes  from  Guidons  hexachord,  and  this  also 
shews  that  he  used  Pythagorean  intonation, 
meaning  in  our  notation: 
/ 


Ffaut 

tonus 
G  sol  re  ut 

tonus 
a  la  mi  re 

semitonium 
bfa 

apothome 
B  mi  Trittonus 

semitonium 
0  sol  fa  ut 


204  cents  s  Tone 

204  cents  a  Tone 
a 

90  cents  »  Semitone 
6b 

1 14  cents  »  Apotome 
h  Tritone  612  cents 

90  cents  a  Semitone 
c'  Fifth  702  cents 


The  Germans  generally  speak  of  b  and  S 
as  Be,  Ereuz  (cross).  I  do  not  remember  ever 
having  heard  the  I)  named,  but  I  find  in  Flii- 
gers  Dictionary  Beqiuidra/twn  (square  b)  and 
Wiederhersiellungazeichen  (sign  of  restitution, 
for  which  Q  was  not  used  till  the  seventeenth 
century).  Germans  never  have  occasion  to  use 
the  word,  because,  instead  of  ^d  flat,  d  natural, 
d  sharp,'  they  say  *  des,  de,  dis,'  while  &b,  6  Q 
are  termed  '  be,  ha.*  On  older  organ  pipes  b  4 
are  constantly  used  for  &b  6  K ,  and  some  organ* 
builders  still  use  them. — Translator,'] 

♦  [The  general  relations  on  which  the 
schemes  of  temperai;^ent  depend  will  be  found 
in  App.  XX.  sect,  k.— Translator.] 

f  [The  cents  in  the  Table  were  of  course 
inserted  by  me. — Translator.] 

Digitized  by  VjOOQIC 


3M 


TEMPEBED  INTONATION. 


PABTXn. 


are  much  more  disagreeable.  For  the  Pythagorean  Thirds  V+e'  and  d—(f  the 
combinational  tones  are  nearly  C)|;  and  B,,  both  differing  by  a  Semitone  &om  the 
combinational  tone  C»  which  woiiLd  result  from  the  perfect  intervals  in  both  cases. 
For  the  Pythagorean  minor  chord  e'—g'-^-h'  the  combinational  tones  are  B,  and 
very  nearly  (rj.  The  first,  J5y,  is  very  suitable,  better  even  than  the  combina- 
tional tone  G  which  resxdts  from  perfect  intonation.  But  the  second,  (?$,  belongs 
to  the  major  and  not  to  the  minor  chord  of  E.  However,  as  in  perfect  intonation 
one  of  the  two  combinational  tones  C  and  Q  is  fedse,  the  Pythagorean  minor  chord 
can  hardly  be  considered  inferior  in  this  respect.  But  the  combinational  tones  of 
the  equally  tempered  Thirds  lie  between  those  of  the  perfect  and  Pythagorean 
Thirds,  and  are  less  than  a  Semitone  different  horn  those  of  just  intonation.  Hence 
they  correspond  to  no  possible  modulation,  no  tone  of  the  chromatic  scale,  no  dis- 
sonance that  could  possibly  be  introduced  by  the  progression  of  the  melody ;  they 

IT  simply  sound  out  of  tune  and  wrong.* 

These  bad  combinational  tones  have  always  been  to  me  the  most  annoying 
part  of  equally  tempered  harmonies.  When  moderately  slow  passages  in  Thirds 
at  rather  a  high  pitch  are  played,  they  form  a  horrible  bass  to  them,  which  is  all 
the  more  disagreeable  for  coming  tolerably  near  to  the  correct  bass,  and  henea 
sounding  as  if  they  were  played  on  some  other  instrument  which  was  dreadiiilly 
out  of  tune.  They  are  heard  most  distinctly  on  the  harmonium  and  violin.  Here 
every  professional  and  even  every  amateur  musician  observes  them  immediately, 
when  their  attention  is  properly  directed.  And  when  the  ear  has  once  become 
accustomed  to  note  them,  it  can  even  discover  them  on  the  piano.  In  the 
Pythagorean  intonation  the  combinational  tones  sound  rather  as  if  some  one  were 
intentionally  playing  dissonances.  Which  of  these  two  evils  is  the  worse,  I  will 
not  venture  to  decide.  In  lower  positions  where  the  very  low  combinati<mal  tones 
can  be  scarcely,  if  at  all,  heard,  the  equally  tempered  Thirds  have  the  advantage 

%  over  the  Greek,  because  they  are  not  so  rough,  and  produce  fewer  beats.  In. 
higher  positions  the  latter  advantage  is  perhaps  destroyed  by  their  combinational 
tones.  However,  the  equally  tempered  system  is  capable  of  effecting  everything 
that  can  be  done  by  the  Pythagorean,  and  with  less  expenditure  of  means. 

C.  E.  Naumann,t  who  has  lately  defended  the  Pythagorean  as  opposed  to  the 
equally  tempered  system,  grounds  his  reasons  chiefly  on  the  fact  that  the  Semi- 
tones which  separate  the  ascending  leading  tone  from  the  tonic,  and  the  descend- 
ing minor  Seventh  from  the  Third  of  the  chord  on  which  it  has  to  be  resolved,  are 
smaller  in  the  Pythagorean  (where  they  are  about  ^ ;  as  appears  in  the  Table  on 
P*  3^3^)  than  in  the  equally  tempered,  where  they  are  about  ||;  while  they  are 
greatest  of  all  in  just  intonation,  viz.  \i.  Now  in  the  equally  tempered  scale  there 
is  only  one  tone  between  /  and  g,  which  is  accepted  at  one  time  as  /jf  to  be  a 
leading  note  to  ^,  and  at  another  as  g\^  to  act  as  a  Seventh  resolving  upon  /;  but 
in  the  Pythagorean  there  are  two  tones, /{);  and  g\},  of  which  the  latter  is  the  flatter. 

%  *  [This  may  be  seen  more  clearly  by  oalcalating  the  pitch  numbers,  assmning  (/  to  be 
264  as  on  p.  17.    Then — 


Notes 

Just,  Difference 

Pythagorean,  Difference 

Tempered,  Difference 

c' 

264 

66«C 
330 

66«C 
396 

99«Qf 

495 

264 

7012 

334*12 

6r88  =  J5, 
396 

105-19 
501-19 

264 

68-61 
332*61 

62-94 
395*55 

I02-8I 

49836 

The  *  differences'  give  the  pitch  nmnbers  of 
the  combinational  tones.  Now  we  have  by 
p.  17a,  C  =  66,  G  «  99,  B^«6r88,  but  the  others 
correspond  to  no  precise  tones.  The  nearest 
equally  tempered  intervals  are  B  62-3,   Off 


« 69-93,  ^^^  ^t  =  104-76. — TranslatoT.\ 

t  Veher  die  verschiedenen  Bestimmungen 
der  Tonverhdltnisse,  [On  the  various  deter- 
minations of  the  ratios  of  tones.]  Leipzig, 
1858. 


Digitized  by  V^OOQIC 


CHAP.  XVI.  TEMPERED  INTONATICttl.  315 

Hence  the  Semitone  always  approaches  the  tone  on  to  which  it  would  fall  in  regular 
resolution,  and  the  height  of  the  pitch  determines  the  direction  of  resolution.  But 
although  the  leading  tone  plays  an  important  part  in  modulations,  it  is  perfectly 
clear  that  we  are  not  justified  in  changing  its  pitch  at  will  in  order  to  bring  it 
nearer  to  the  note  on  which  it  has  to  be  resolved.  There  would  otherwise  be  no 
limit  to  our  making  it  come  nearer  and  nearer  to  that  tone,  as  in  the  ancient  Greek 
enharmonic  mode.*  Suppose  we  replace  the  Pythagorean  Semitone,  which  is 
about  f  of  the  natural  Semitone,  by  another  still  smaller,  about  f  of  the  natural 
one,  say  44  x  |^  x  |^ ;  the  result  would  be  perfectly  unnatural  as  a  leading  note.f 
We  have  already  seen  that  the  character  of  the  leading  note  essentially  depends 
upon  its  being  that  tone  in  the  scale  which  is  most  distantly  related  to  the  tonic, 
and  hence  most  uncertain  and  alterable  [melodically].  Hence  we  are  perfectly 
unjustified  in  deducing  from  such  a  tone  the  principle  of  construction  for  the  whole 
scale.  ^ 

The  principal  fault  of  our  present  tempered  intonation,  therefore,  does  not  lie 
in  the  Fifths ;  for  their  imperfection  is  really  not  worth  speaking  of,  and  is 
scarcely  perceptible  in  chords.  The  fault  rather  lies  in  the  Thirds,  and  this  error 
is  not  due  to  forming  the  Thirds  by  means  of  a  series  of  imperfect  Fifths,  but  it  is 
the  old  Pythagorean  error  of  forming  the  Thirds  by  means  of  an  ascending  series  of 
four  Fifths.  Perfect  Fifths  in  this  case  give  even  a  worse  result  than  flat  Fifths. 
The  natural  relation  of  the  major  Third  to  the  tonic,  both  melodically  and  harmo- 
nically, depends  on  the  ratio  f  of  the  pitch  numbers.  Any  other  Third  is  only  a 
more  or  less  unsatisfactory  substitute  for  the  natural  major  Third.  The  only 
correct  system  of  tones  is  that  in  which,  as  Hauptmann  proposed,  the  system  of 
tones  generated  by  Fifths  should  be  separated  from  those  generated  by  major 
Thirds.  Now  as  it  is  important  for  the  solution  of  many  theoretical  questions  to 
be  able  to  make  experiments  on  tones  which  really  form  with  each  other  the 
natural  intervals  required  by  theory,  to  prevent  the  ear  from  being  deceived  by  IT 
the  imperfections  of  the  equal  temperament,  I  have  endeavoured  to  have  an  in- 
strument constructed  by  which  I  could  modulate  by  perfect  intervals  into  all  keys. 

If  we  were  really  obliged  to  produce  in  all  its  completeness  the  system  of  tones 
distinguished  by  Hauptmann,  in  order  to  obtain  perfect  intervals  in  all  keys,  it 
would  certainly  be  scarcely  possible  to  overcome  the  diflBculties  of  the  problem. 
Fortunately  it  is  possible  to  introduce  a  great  and  essential  simplification  by  means 
of  the  artifice  originally  invented  by  the  Arabic  and  Persian  musicians,  and  pre- 
viously mentioned  on  p.  281a. 

We  have  afready  seen  that  the  tones  of  Hauptmann's  system  which  are  generated 
by  Fifths,  and  are  marked  by  letters  without  any  subscribed  or  superscribed  lines,  as 
c±:gf±^±a±,  Ac,  are  one  comma  or  f|  [=22  cents]  higher  than  the  notes  which 
bear  the  same  names,  when  generated  by  major  Thirds,  and  which  are  here  dis- 
tinguished by  an  inferior  figure  as  Cj  ±gf  1  ±(Zi  ±ai  ±,  &c.  We  have  further  seen  that 
if  we  descend  from  6  by  a  series  of  12  Fifths  down  to  ct>,  the  last  tone,  reduced  to  IT 
the  proper  Octave,  is  lower  than  b  by  about  fj  [=24  cents].    Hence  we  have — 

b  :  5,=8i  :  80 
b  :  ct>=74  :  73 

Now  these  two  intervals  are  very  nearly  alike ;  bi  is  rather  higher  than  c\},  but 
only  in  the  proportion — 

c\}  :  51=32768  :  32805  [=2  cents] 

*  [However  justifiable  BDch  alterations  may  place  of   112  cents).    No  diminution  of  the 

be  in  unaccompanied  melody,  they  are  de-  just  Semitone  can  be  made  without  injury  to 

Btructive  of  harmony,  and  hence  do  not  belong  the  major  Thirds. — Translator.] 
to  harmonic  music  proper.    Of  all  the  older  f  [This  would  have  112  — 2x22^68  cents, 

temperaments,    the  meantone   is    most  har-  which  approaches  very  closely  to  the  small 

monious,  but  this  makes  the  leading  tones  Semitone  25  :  24970  cents,  so  that  the  effect 

still  further  from  the  tone  on  which  they  are  can  be  judged  from  playing  &'b...&i  on  the 

resolved,  than  even  in  just  intonation  (117  in  Harmonical.— 2Va»wZator.] 


Digitized  by 


\^oogle 


3i6 


HABMONIUM  IN  JUST  INTONATION. 


PABT  UI. 


or,  using  the  approximation  obtained  by  continued  fractiouB — 

c|>  :  61=885  :  886. 

The  interval  between  ct>  and  bi  is  consequently  about  the  same  as  that  between 
a  perfect  and  an  equally  tempered  Fifth.* 

Now  bi  is  the  true  major  Third  of  g,  and  if  we  descend  8  Fifths  from  g  we 
arrive  at  c\}  thus  : 

g±c±f±b\}±e\}±a\}±d\}±g\}±c\} 

Now,  as  c\}  is  flatter  than  &i,  if  we  diminished  f  all  the  Fifths  by  i  of  the  small 
interval  fff  we  should  arrive  at  &i  instead  of  c\}. 

Now,  since  the  interval  f|f  is  itself  on  the  limits  of  sensible  difference  of  pitch, 
the  eighth  part  of  this  interval  cannot  be  taken  into  account  at  all,  and  we  may 
1  consequently  identify  the  following  tones  of  Hauptmann's  system,  by  proceeding  in 
a  series  of  Fifths  from  c[>=&i,  that  is,  the  upper  line  with  the  lower,  or — 

f\}±c\}±g\}  ±d\}  ±a\}  ±e\}  ±b\> 
:=e,±b,  ±/J±c,#±^,#±d,#±aj# 

Among  musical  instruments,  the  harmonium,  on  account  of  its  uniformly  sus- 
tained sound,  the  piercing  character  of  its  quality  of  tone,  and  its  tolerably  distinct 
combinational  tones,  is  particularly  sensitive  to  inaccuracies  of  intonation.  And  as 
its  vibrators  also  admit  of  a  delicate  and  durable  tuning,  it  appeared  to  me  pecu- 
liarly suitable  for  experiments  on  a  more  perfect  system  of  tones.  I  therefore 
selected  an  harmonium  of  the  larger  kind,  j:  with  two  manuals,  and  a  set  of  vibrators 
for  each,  and  had  it  so  tuned  that  by  using  the  tones  of  the  two  manuals  I  could 
play  all  the  major  chords  from  JP]>  major  to  Fjj^  major.  The  tones  are  thus  dia-- 
f  tributed : 


/b  +  a,b-cb  +  e,b-gb  +  6,b— <ib+/,- 
On  Lower  Manual 


eb  +  flfi  -  6b  +  i|  - /+ a,-|-c 

„•; II fj D 

On  Upper  Manual 


I     II     II ^^1 

On  Lower  Manual 


-e+g^U  -6  +  d,«  -Jt  +a,J  -cj  + 
On  Upper  Manual 


eiS 


This  instrument  therefore  furnishes  15  major  chords  and  as  many  minor 
chords,  with  perfectly  pure  Thirds,  but  with  Fifths  too  flat  by  i  of  the  interval  by 
which  an  equally  tempered  Fifth  is  too  flat.§    On  the  Lower  Manual  we  have  the 


*  [On  aooount  of  the  approximate  character 
ol  the  calculation,  the  extreme  closeness  of 
result  is  not  well  shewn.  Taking  the  accurate 
f  numbers,  the  ratio 

^+cb«S?i{.  giving  cents  r95372i. 

Perfect  Fifth + tempered  Fifth  ^f+'i'/, 
giving  cents  701  •955001  — 700  « I -955001.  Dif- 
ference *ooi28o  cents.  Human  ears,  however 
much  assisted  by  human  contrivances,  could 
never  hear  the  difference. — Translator.] 

f  [Accidentally  misprinted  *  increased,*  that 
is,  *  zu  gross,'  and  '  too  sharp,*  that  is,  *  za 
hoch,*  instead  of  *  zu  klein,*  and  *  zu  tief  *  in 
all  the  four  Oerman  editions.  This  error 
evidently  arose  merely  from  forgetting  for  the 
moment  that  the  Fifths  were  taken  down  and 
not  ttjp.  Now  8  perfect  Fifths  down  ■■  —  8  x  702 
cents  »  —  5616  cents,  which  on  adding  5  octaves 
b6ooo  cents,  gives  384  cents,  and  this  is  less 
than  the  major  Thirds  of  386  cents  by  2  cents. 
Hence  if  we  diminish  each  Fifth  by  i  cent, 
8  diminished  Fifths  down  «  —  8  x  7oi|  =  —  5614 
cents,  which,  on  adding  5  octaves  or  0000  cents 


gives  386  cents,  and  this  is  the  correct  major 
Third.  But  to  tune  Fifths  of  this  kind,  if  pos- 
sible, would  be  a  work  of  immense  labour  even 
with  tuning-forks,  the  most  permanent  of  exist- 
ing conveyors  of  pitch,  and  the  most  perfeot 
apparatus  known.  Thus  such  a  Fifth  reckoned 
from  cf  264  vib.  gives  ^  395*944  vib.,  while  the 
perfect  g'  is  396*000  vib.,  difference  '056  vib., 
which  it  is  hopeless  to  tune  exactly.  Henoe 
these  Fifths  can  only  be  regarded  as  products 
of  calculation  which  could  not  be  realised.  In 
App.  XX.  sect  A.  art.  18  I  term  the  result- 
ing temperament  Helmholtzian,  although,  as 
will  be  seen  in  the  following  note  §,  Prof. 
Helmholtz  himself  did  not  attempt  to  realise 
it. — TranslatorJ] 

X  Made  by  Messrs.  J.  &  P*  Schiedmayer, 
in  Stuttgart. 

§  The  tuning  of  this  instrument  was  easily 
managed.  Herr  Schiedmayer  succeeded  at  the 
first  attempt  by  the  following  direction.  Start- 
ing  from  a  on  the  lower  manual,  tune  the 
Fifths  d±a,g±dtC±g  perfectly  just,  and  thus 


Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


OHAP.  XYI. 


HABMONIUM  IN  JUST  INTONATION. 


317 


complete  scales  of  C\>  major  and  O  major  and  in  the  upper  the  complete  scales  of 
II\}  major  and  B  major  inclusive  complete.  All  the  major  scales  exist  from  C\} 
major  to  B  major,  and  they  can  all  be  played  with  perfect  exactness  in  the  natural 
intonation.  But  to  modulate  beyond  B  major  on  the  one  side  and  C\}  major  on  the 
other,  it  is  necessary  to  make  a  really  enharmonic  interchange  between  B^  and  Ct>, 
which  perceptibly  alters  the  pitch  (by  a  comma  |^).*  The  minor  modes  on  the 
lower  manual  are  Bi  minor  or  G\}  minor  complete,  on  the  upper  manual  DiJj^  minor 
or  E\}  minor. 

For  the  minor  keys  this  series  of  tones  is  not  quite  so  satisfactory  as  for  the 
major  keys.  The  dominant  of  a  minor  key  is  the  Fifth  of  a  minor  triad  and  the 
root  of  a  major  triad.  But  as  the  minor  chord  has  to  be  written  as  ai  —  c  +  ^i, 
and  the  major  chord  as  / 1>  +  ^i  [>  —  c[>,  the  corresponding  dominant  must  be  written 
in  the  first  chord  with  an  inferior  number,  and  in  the  second  with  a  letter  without 
any  number  attached ;  that  is,  they  must  be  tones  of  the  kind  which  we  have  ^ 
identified  by  means  of  the  assumptions  here  made,  as  in  the  present  case  where  Ci 


obtain  c,  g,  d.  Then  tone  the  major  chords 
c  +  e,— <7,  g  +  b^—d.d  +  fji  -a,  giving  the  Ihred 
tones  ei&i/iS  1  and  finally  the  Fifth,  fit  ±  CjS , 
to  obtain  CiS,  Then  patting  e^^fb^  2>i«cb, 
/18  "I7b»  c,S  adbi  tune  the  major  chords 
/b+a,b-cb,  cb  +  eib-S'b,  gb-^-h^b-db  with 
pnre  Thirds  giving  no  beats,  thas  obtaining 
a,  b,  ei  b,  &i  b ,  and  finally  the  Fifth  5|  b  ±/i >  giving 
/,.  This  completes  the  tuning  of  the  notes  on 
the  lower  manual.  For  the  upper  manual  first 
tune  e  as  the  perfect  Fifth  of  the  a  in  the 
lower  manual,  and  then  the  three  major  chords 
e  +  Jf,!  -6,6  +  d,»  -/5,/5  +a,5  -c»,andthe 
Fifth a,»  ±e.J,giving6,y5,clf  ,andthengr,5, 
d|8,  OiS  and  also  e^^.  Then  put  g^%  -ab, 
d|S  -eb,a|8  »6b,  e^t  »/,  and  tune  the  Thirds 
in  the  major  chords  ab  +  c,~eb»  eb  +  gi  — &b, 
6b+di-/,  and  the  Fifth  d^jto^.  This  gives 
^1*  fif  ^n  <^^  ^1*  <^^  completes  the  whole 
tuning,  which  is  much  easier  than  for  a  series 
of  equally  tempered  tones. 

[The  theoretical  flattening  of  all  the  Fifths 
by  )  of  a  skhisma  is  here  neglected,  as  it 
woiJd  be  impossible  by  ear  only,  and  in  all 
probability  many  other  errors  in  tuning  were 
eommitted,  which  could  not  be  detected.  The 
result  is  that  the  two  manuals  were  tuned  to 
the  following  tones,  using  capital  letters  to 


represent  the  large  or  white  digitals,  and  the 
small  letters  the  small  or  black  digitals.  The 
Boman  letters  below  shew  the  secondary  mean- 
ing attached  to  the  letters  above  them  for  the 
tuning  of  the  notes  marked  with  a  *  above 
them. 

Upper  ManuaL 

61  c«  A  d,t  E  E,t  ft  G,  g,t  J,  a,«  B 
eb         f  ab         bb 

Lower  Manual. 

0  Cj«    D  «I>  E,  F,  /,5   G  Jjb  A  6*b  B, 
db  Fb        gb  Cb 

To  make  it  more  dear  how  the  24  notes  of  ^ 
this  instmment  represent  48  by  neglecting  the 
skhisma,  I  have  below  arranged  the  scale  on 
the  duodenary  system  (major  Thirds  in  lines. 
Fifths  in  columns,  App.  XX.  sect.  E.  art.  18), 
and  given  the  proper  number  of  cents  for  each 
note,  using  capitals  for  the  notes  actually 
tuned,  and  smiUl  letters  for  those  obtained  by 
substitution.  The  notes  above  the  horizontal 
line  were  in  the  upper  manual,  those  below  it 
on  the  lower. 
It  is  thus  seen  i)  that  the  notes  in  cols.  L,  II., 


L 

II. 

in. 

IV. 

V. 

VL 

Ct   114 
Ft  612 
B  mo 
E    408 

E,t  500 
4.«  998 
D,J  296 

Oit  794 

gjit 

886 

184 
682 
1 180 

d'b        112 

0*b        610 
c'b       iio8 
fb        406 

/      498 
6b    996 
eb    294 
ab    792 

B,  182 
G,    680 

C,  I 178 

A     906 
D     204 
G     702 
C        0 

J^it  590 
B,  1088 
E,    386 

gji 

478 

976 

274 
772 

6»bb      904 
e»bb      202 
a»bb      700 
d'bb    1198 

db     90 
gb    588 
cb  1086 
/b    384 

F,    476 
B,b974 
E^b  272 
A,b  770 

III.are  exactly  2  cents  sharper  than  those  in  cols. 
IV.,  v.,  VI.  2)  that  only  cols.  I.,  H.,  VI.  were 
tuned,  and  that  IV.,  V.,  and  HE.  without  being 
tuned  were  assumed  to  be  identical  with  them 
respectively.  3)  That  cols.  I.,  II.,  III.  form  a 
series  of  Fifths  down  or  Fourths  up,  of  which 
only  two,  namely  C  to  ^,S  and  E^  to  9^  S , 
are  defective,  being  both  700  cents  down  or 
500  up,  in  place  of  702  and  498  as  all  the 
others.  4)  That  the  simplest  way  of  tuning 
would  be  to  take  A  to  pitch,  and  then  A  +  C,S, 
and  Cit  +  ejS  as  perfect  major  Thirds,  and  then 
from  A,  Cit ,  e^t  to  tune  the  rest  of  the  notes 


in  their  columns  by  perfect  Fifths  and  Fourths, 
naming  the  notes  in  col.  IIL  for  convenience  as 
those  in  col.  VI.  Afterwards  the  identity  of  the 
first  three  with  the  last  three  columns  would 
be  assumed.  All  the  properties  and  defects 
of  this  system  of  tuning  can  be  immediately 
deduced  from  the  above  diagram.— TraYuZa- 
tor.] 

*  [For  instead  of  the  keys  of  J^  and  JPb, 
the  absence  of  Gt^  and  6b b,  <2,b  obliges  us 
to  use  the  keys  of  F,8  and  F^b,  which  are 
respectively  a  comma  lower  and  higher. — 
Translator,] 

Digitized  by  V^OOQIC 


3i8  HARMONIUM  IN  JUST  INTONATION.  part  irf. 

is  identified  with  /[>•  Hence  the  inBtniment  famishes  the  following  eight  perfectly 
just  minor  scales  [where  the  letters  in  brackets  indicate  those  which  are  not  written 
in  the  account  of  the  manuals  in  the  text  of  p.  316c] : — 

i)  ai   or  b\}\}  minor :  di  —  f    +  ai    —  c  -h  ei    +  \gjj^   --  ^i  ] 

f\}  +  a,b    -  c\} 

2)  Bi    or/t)    minor:  ay  ^  c    -h «!    -^  +  6,    +  [djj^  -/i#] 

3)  6,   orct>    minor:  Bi    -g    -f  ^1    -d  +/i#+  [^ajf  -  CiiO 

g\}  +  6,>    -  d\} 
4)/i#orgrt>    minor:  ii  -  d   +/,# -a  +  c,#  +  [«2#    -  9\9\ 

d\>  +  fi       -a\> 
5)Ci#ord|>    minor  :/,#-.  a   +  cj  -  «  +  ^J  +  [^a#  -  ^i#] 
IT  at>'  +  Ci       —  el> 

6)  ^,#or  a^    minor :  Ci#-  6    +  grj  -  6  +  dj  +  [/jfttf-  «i#] 

«>   +  ^1       -  ^t> 

7)  di#  or  eb    minor :  gf, J-  6    +  d,#  -/J  +  a,#  +  [cJJ  -  e,#] 

^b  +  d,       -/ 

8)  aij;  or  6t>    minor :  dij-yj  +  a,#  -  cjf  +  c,#  +  [Sfafttf-  *iftl 

/     +  a,       -  c 

Of  these,  the  six  last  tonics  from  C|>  to  B\}  are  also  provided  with  major  scales. 
Hence  there  are  complete  minor  scales  on  all  degrees  of  the  scales  of  Bi  major  and 
^1  major ;  and  complete  minor  and  major  scales  on  all  degrees  of  the  scale  of  Bi 
major,  with  the  exception  of  Ei. 

After  previous  experiments  on  another  harmonium,  where  I  had  at  command 
only  the  two  sets  of  tones  of  one  octave  common  to  two  stops  with  one  manual,  I 

f  had  expected  that  it  would  be  scarcely  observed  if  either  the  other  minor  keys  had 
a  somewhat  too  sharp  Pythagorean  Seventh,  or  if  minor  chords  which  are  them- 
selves rather  obscurely  harmonious,  were  executed  in  Pythagorean  intonation. 
When  isolated  minor  chords  are  struck  the  difference  is,  indeed,  not  much  observed. 
But  when  long  series  of  justly-intoned  chords  have  been  employed,  and  the  ear  has 
grown  accustomed  to  their  effect,  it  becomes  so  sensitive  to  any  intermixture  of 
chords  in  imperfect  intonation,  that  the  disturbance  is  very  appreciable.* 

The  least  disturbance  is  caused  by  taking  the  Pythagorean  Seventh,  because 
this  leading  tone  is  in  modem  compositions  scarcely  ever  used  but  in  the  chord  of 
the  dominant  Seventh,  or  other  dissonances.  In  a  pure  major  triad  its  effect  is 
certainly  very  harsh.  But  in  a  discord  it  has  a  less  disturbing  effect,  becau^  by 
its  sharpness  it  brings  out  the  character  of  the  leading  note  more  distinctly.  On 
the  other  hand,  I  have  found  minor  chords  with  Pythagorean  Thirds  absolutely 
intolerable  when  coming  between  justly-intoned  major  and  nunor  chords.*    By 

^  allowing,  then,  a  Pythagorean  Seventh  in  the  scale,  or  a  Pythagorean  major  Third 
in  the  chord  of  the  dominant  Seventh,  we  may  form  the  following  minor  scales :  t — 

9)  di  minor :  gr,     —  fc[>  +  dj    — /     +  aj    ...  cS  I  «i 

10)  gi  minor :  Ci     —  eb  +  ^1    —  6i>  +  dj    .../iff  I  ai 

11)  Ci  minor :  /j     —  at>  +  Ci    —  et>  +  fiTi    ...  61    I  di 

12)  /,  minor :  6,l>  —  dt>  +  /i     —  at>  +  Cj    ...  e,    I  ^, 

13)  6,t>  minor  :  a,t>  —  {/b  +  *il>  —  ^t>  +  /i     •••  ^1    I  ^1 

14)  e,b  minor  :  a^]}  —  c^  +  Cjb  —  g\}  +  ^\\>  •••  ^1    1  /i 

*  [My  own  experience  is  that  the  minor  ments  enable  me  to  compare    these  effects 

chords  even  more  than  the  major  shew  the  readily,  and  both  arise  from  similar,  though 

vast  superiority  of  the  just  intonation  over  the  not  the  same  causes.— TranaZafor.] 
equal  temperament;  and  that  the  occasional  f  [In  which  (...)  represents  the  Pythago- 

introduction  of    Pythagorean  among    justly-  rean  major  Third  of  408  cents  and  (  |  )  the 

intoned  chords,  major  or  minor,  is  comparable  Pythagorean    minor    Third    of   294  cents.— 


only  to  the  •  wolves  *  on  the  *  bad  keys,*  as  Eb,      Translator,] 
or  E,  of  the  old  organ  tuning.    My  instru- 


Digitized  by 


Google 


CHAP.  XVI.  HAEMONIUM  IN  JUST  INTONATION.  319 

In  the  former  series,  Nos.  8  and  7,  we  had  already  b\}  minor  and  e\^  minor, 
which  are  a  comma  sharper  than  Nos.  13  and  14.  Hence  the  series  of  minor 
keys  is  also  completed  by  the  fusion  of  their  extremities  through  enharmonic  inter^ 
change. 

In  most  cases  it  is  possible  to  transpose  the  music  to  be  played  on  such  instru- 
ments, so  as  to  avoid  the  necessity  of  making  these  enharmonic  interchanges,  pro- 
vided the  modulations  do  not  extend  too  far  into  different  keys.  But  if  it  is  not 
possible  to  avoid  enharmonic  interchanges,  they  must  be  introduced  where  two 
unrelated  *  chords  follow  each  other.  This  is  best  done  between  dissonant  chords. 
Naturally  this  enharmonic  change  is  always  necessary  when  a  piece  of  music 
modulates  through  the  whole  circle  of  Fifths — from  C  major  to  Bjj^  major,  for 
example.  But  Hauptmann  is  certainly  right  when  he  characterises  such  circular 
modulation  as  unnatural  artificiality,  which  could  only  be  rendered  possible  by  the 
imperfections  of  our  modem  system  of  temperament.  Such  a  process  must  cer-  IT 
tainly  destroy  the  hearer's  feeling  for  the  unity  of  the  tonic.  For  although  Bjj^ 
has  very  nearly  the  same  pitch  as  0,  or  can  be  even  improperly  identified  with  it, 
the  hearer  can  only  restore  his  feeling  for  the  former  tonic  by  going  back  on  the 
same  path  that  he  advanced.  He  cannot  possibly  retain  his  recollection  of  the 
absolute  pitch  of  the  first  tonic  C  after  his  long  modulations  up  to  ^,  with  such 
a  degree  of  exactness  as  to  be  able  to  recognise  that  they  are  identical.  For  any 
fine  artistic  feeling  Bjj^  must  remain  a  tonic  far  removed  from  G  on  the  dominant 
side ;  or,  more  probably,  after  such  distant  modulations,  the  hearer's  whole  feeling 
for  tonality  will  have  become  confased,  and  it  will  then  be  perfectly  indifferent  to 
him  in  what  key  the  piece  ends.  Generally  speaking,  an  immoderate  use  of  strik- 
ing modulations  is  a  suitable  and  easy  instrument  in  the  hands  of  modem  com- 
posers, to  make  their  pieces  piquant  and  highly  coloured.  But  a  man  cannot  live 
upon  spice,  and  the  consequence  of  restless  modulation  is  almost  always  the 
obliteration  of  artistic  connection.  It  must  not  be  forgotten  that  modulations  f 
should  be  only  a  means  of  giving  prominence  to  the  tonic  by  contrasting  it  with 
another  and  then  returning  into  it,  or  of  attaining  isolated  and  peculiar  effects  of 
expression. 

Since  harmoniums  with  two  manuals  have  usually  two  sets  of  vibrators  for 
each  manual  of  which  the  above  system  of  tuning  only  uses  one,  I  have  had  the 
two  others  (an  8-foot  and  a  16-foot  stop)  tuned  in  the  usual  equal  temperament, 
which  renders  it  very  easy  to  compare  the  effect  of  this  tuning  with  just  intona- 
tion, as  I  have  merely  to  pull  out  or  push  in  a  stop  to  make  the  difference.f 

As  regards  musical  effect,  the  difference  between  the  just  and  the  equally- 
tempered,  or  the  just  and  the  Pythagorean  intonations,  is  very  remarkable.  The 
justly-intoned  chords,  in  &vourable  positions,  notwithstanding  the  rather  piercing 
quality  of  the  tone  of  the  vibrators,  possess  a  fall  and  as  it  were  saturated  har- 
moniousness ;  they  flow  on,  with  a  full  stream,  calm  and  smooth,  without  tremor 
or  beat.  Equally- tempered  or  Pythagorean  chords  sound  beside  them  rough,  dull,  f 
trembling,  restless.  The  difference  is  so  marked  that  every  one,  whether  he  is 
musically  cultivated  or  not,  observes  it  at  once.  Chords  of  the  dominant  Seventh 
in  just  intonation  have  nearly  the  same  degree  of  roughness  as  a  common  major 
chord  of  the  same  pitch  in  tempered  intonation.  The  difference  between  natural 
and  tempered  intonation  is  greatest  and  most  unpleasant  in  the  higher  Octaves  of 
the  scale,  because  here  the  false  combinational  tones  of  the  tempered  intonation 
are  more  observable,  and  the  number  of  beats  for  equal  differences  of  pitch  becomes 
larger,  and  hence  the  roughness  greater. 

A  second  circumstance  of  essential  importance  is,  that  the  differences  of  effect 
between  major  and  minor  chords,  between  different  inversions  and  positions  of 

*  [That  is,  chords  not  haying  a  common  and  at  the    same  time    greatly  facilitating 

tone. — Translator.]  ^  fingering  by  the  use  of  a  single  manual,  will 

f  Proposals  for  making  the  series  of  tones  be  found  in  Appendix  XVII. 
in  this  system  of  intonation  more  complete 


Digitized  by 


Google 


32Q  HARMONIUM  IN  JUST  INTONATION.  pabtiii. 

chords  of  the  same  kind,  and  between  consonances  and  dissonances  are  much 
more  decided  and  conspicuous,  than  in  the  equal  temperament.  Hence  modu- 
lations become  much  more  expressive.  Many  fine  distinctions  are  sensible,  which 
otherwise  almost  disappear,  as,  for  instance,  those  which  depend  on  the  different 
inversions  and  positions  of  chords,  while,  on  the  Other  hand,  the  intensity  of  the 
harsher  dissonances  is  much  increased  by  their  contrast  with  perfect  chords.  The 
chord  of  the  diminished  Seventh,  for  example,  which  is  so  much  used  in  modem 
music,  borders  upon  the  insupportable,  when  the  other  chords  are  tuned  justly.* 

Modem  musicians  who,  with  rare  exceptions,  have  never  heard  any  mnsio 
executed  except  in  equal  temperament,  mostly  make  light  of  the  inexactness  of 
tempered  intonation.  The  errors  of  the  Fifths  are  very  small.  There  is  no  doubt 
of  that.  And  it  is  usual  to  say  that  the  Thirds  are  much  less  perfect  consonances 
than  the  Fifths,  and  consequently  also  less  sensitive  to  errors  of  intonation.    Tlie 

f  last  assertion  is  also  correct,  so  long  as  homophonic  music  is  considered,  in  which 
the  Thirds  occur  only  as  melodic  intervals  and  not  in  harmonic  combinations.  In 
a  consonant  triad  every  tone  is  equally  sensitive  to  false  intonation,  as  theory  and 
experience  alike  testify,  and  the  bad  effect  of  the  tempered  triads  depends  especially 
on  the  imperfect  Thirds.f 

There  can  be  no  question  that  the  simplicity  of  tempered  intonation  is  ex* 
tremely  advantageous  for  instrumental  music,  that  any  other  intonation  requires 
an  extraordinarily  greater  complication  in  the  mechanism  of  the  instrument,  and 
would  materially  increase  the  difficulties  of  manipulation,  and  that  consequently 
the  high  development  of  modem  instrumental  music  would  not  have  been  possible 
without  tempered  intonation.  But  it  must  not  be  imagined  that  the  difference 
between  tempered  and  just  intonation  is  a  mere  mathematical  subtilty  without  any 
practical  value.  That  this  difference  is  really  very  striking  even  to  unmuaieal  ears, 
is  shewn  immediately  by  actual  experiments  with  properly  tuned  instruments^ 

f  And  that  the  early  musicians,  who  were  still  accustomed  to  the  perfect  intervals  of 
vocal  music,  which  was  then  most  carefully  practised,  felt  the  same,  is  immediately 
seen  by  a  glance  at  the  musical  writings  of  the  latter  half  of  the  seventeenth  and 
the  earlier  part  of  the  eighteenth  centuries,  at  which  time  there  was  much  dis- 
cussion about  the  introduction  of  different  kinds  of  temperament,  and  one  new 
method  after  another  was  invented  and  rejected  for  escaping  the  difficulties,  and 
the  most  ingenious  forms  of  instrument  were  designed  for  practically  executing 
the  enharmonic  differences  of  the  tones.  Praetoriust  mentions  a  universal 
cymbalum,  which  he  saw  at  the  house  of  the  court-organist  of  the  Emperor 
Budolph  n.  in  Prague,  and  which  had  77  digitals  in  4  octaves,  or  19  to  the 
octave,  the  black  digitals  being  doubled,  and  others  inserted  between  those  for  s 
and/,  and  between  those  for  b  and  c.§  In  the  older  directions  for  tuning,  several 
tones  are  usually  tuned  by  Fifths  which  beat  slightly,  and  then  others  as  perfect 
major  Thirds.    The  intervals  on  which  the  errors  accumulated  were  called  wolves*. 

^       *  [This  should  be  tried  on  the  Hannonioal  where  the  Fifth  is  only  one-eleventh  of  a 

as  h^'-d\f-a}\>.    Although  it  has  two  per-  comma  or  2  cents  too  flat,  and  tke  major 

feet  minor  Thirds  and  only  one  Pythagorean,  Third  is  seven-elevenths  of  a  comma  or  14 

it  is  a  mere  piece  of  noise,  of  a  much  worse  cents  too  sharp,  and  hence  the  minor  Third  is 

kind  than  the  noise  of  the  equally-tempered  eight-elevenths  of  a  comma  or  16  cents  too 

imitation  of  the  same    chord  in  the  same  flat.    The  effect  is  much  more  strongly  felt 

quality  of  tone.    In  just  intonation  the  chord  in  playing  passages  than  in  playing  isolated 

of  the  diminished  Seventh  can  therefore  be  chords.— 2Va?wZa<or.] 

used  only  with  mild  qualities  of  tone.    But  %  Syntagma  musicum,  II.,  Chap.  XL,  p. 

the  real  intonation  of  this  chord  is  to  :  12  :  14  63. 

:  17,  which  can  also  be  played  on  the  Har-  §  [This  was  to  make  the  meantone  acala 
monical  as  0/'3i6  ^267  'd^'b  336  "d'^'b. —  more  complete,  the  scale  being  C  cff  db  -D  dt 
Tramlator.-]  e\>  E  el  F  /«  g\>  Q  gt  a)>  A  a«  6b  B  6j  C, 
f  [A  triad  in  which  the  major  Third  is  where  the  capitals  represent  the  white,  and 
perfect,  but  the  Fifth  and  minor  Third  both  small  letters  the  black  digitals,  all  in  mean- 
too  small  by  a  quarter  of  a  comma  or  5}  cents  tone  temperament,  the  effect  of  which  would 
(as  in  the  meantone  temperament,  in  which  I  have  been  very  good  on  the  organ.  For  the 
have  a  concertina  tuned),  has  a  very  much  intonation  of  these  notes  see  App.  XX.  sect.  A. 
better  effect  than  the  equally -tempered  triad,  art.  16, —TransUUor.] 

Digitized  by  VjOOQlC 


CHAP.  XVI.       DISADVANTAGES  OF  TEMPERED  INTONATION.  321 

PraBtorius  says  :  *  It  is  best  for  the  wolf  to  remain  iii  tlie  wood  with  its  abominable 
howling,  and  n6t  disturb  our  hannonicas  concordantias,'     Hameau,  too,  who  at  a 
later  period  contributed  greatly  to  the  introduction  of  equal  temperament,  in  1726  * 
still  defended  a  different  style  of  tuning,  in  which  the  Thirds  of  the  more  usual 
keys  were  kept  perfect  at  the  expense  of  the  Fifths  and  of  the  unusual  keys. 
Thus  he  tuned  up  from  G,  in  Fifths  so  much  diminished,  that  the  fourth  Fifths 
instead  of  being  E,  became  the  perfect  Third  of  C,  namely  ^i=i^t>.    Then  again 
four  Fifths  more  to  i4i|7,  the  perfect  Third  of  F\}t  instead  of  to  A\}.    But  then  the 
four  Fifths  between  this  Ai\}  and  G  had  necessarily  f  to  be  made  too  large,  because 
it  is  not  Ai\}  but  A\}  which  is  four  perfect  Fifths  distant  from  C     This  plan  of 
tuning  gives  the  perfect  major  Thirds,  C+-E,,  G-l-5,,  D-fi^iJ,  -Kj  +  Gajj!,  but 
when  we  proceed  further  from  E  on  the  dominant  side,  or  from  G  on  the  sub- 
dominant  side,  we  find  Thirds  which  become  worse  and  worse.     The  error  in  the 
Fifths  is  about  three  times  that  in  the  equal  temperament.    Even  in  1762,  this  ^ 
system  could  be  characterised  by  d'Alembert  as  that  commonly  used  in  France,  in 
opposition  to  the  equal  temperament  which  Bameau  subsequently  proposed.     Mar- 
purg  t  has  collected  a  long  series  of  other  systems  of  tuning.     Since  players  found 
themselves  compelled  by  the  use  of  only  12  digitals  to  the  octave,  to  put  up  with  a 
series  of  false  intervals,  and  to  let  their  ears  become  accustomed  to  them,  it  was 
certainly  better  to  make  up  their  minds  to  give  up  their  few  perfect  major  Thirds 
Btill  remaimng  in  the  scale,  and  to  make  all  the  major  Thirds  equally  erroneous.   It 
necessarily  produces  more  disturbance  to  hear  very  falsely  tuned  Thirds  amidst 
correct  intervals,  than  to  hear  intervals  which  are  all  equally  out  of  tune  and  are 
not  contrasted  with  others  in  perfect  intonation.    Hence  as  long  as  it  is  necessary 
practically  to  hmit  the  number  of  separate  tones  witliin  the  octave  to  12,  there  caif 
be  no  question  at  all  as  to  the  superiority  of  the  equal  temperament  with  its  12  equal 
Semitones,  over  all  others,  and,  as  a  natural  consequence,  this  has  become  the  sole 
acknowledged  method  of  tuning.     It  is  only  bowed  instruments,  with  [including  ^ 
the  Tenor]  their  four  perfect  Fifths  G±G±D±A±E,  which  still  deviate  from  it. 
The  equal  temperament  came  into  use  in  Germany  before  it  was  introduced  into 
France.    In  the  second  volume  of  Matheson's  Gritica  Mtisicdy  which  appeared  in 
1752,  he  mentions  Neidhard  and  Werckmeister  as  the  inventors  of  this  tempera- 
ment.§     Sebastian  Bach  had  already  used  it  for  the  clavichord  (clavier),  as  we 
must  conclude  from  Marpurg's  report  of  Kimbeirger's  assertion,  that  when  he  was 
a  pupil  of  the  elder  Bach  he  had  been  made  to  tune  all  the  major  Thirds  too  sharp.' 
Sebastian's  son,  Emanuel,  who  was  a  celebrated  pianist,  and  published  in  1753  a 

*  Nouveau  Systime    de   Muaiquet  Chap.  D,  O  on  the  other,    fience  oame  the  wolves. 

XXIV.  And  a  system  of  tuning  was  blamed  for  not 

t  [That  is,  if  only  twelve  digitals  might  doing  what  it  never  professed  to  do.    As  long 

be  used,  so  that  the  temperament    became  as  twelve  digitals  only  are  insisted  on,  the 

unequal.    But  this  style  of  tuning,  which  at  equal   temperament,  by  dividing  the  Octavo 

first  was  the  meantone  temperament,  where  into  twelve  equal   Semitones,  is  a  necessity. 

the  Fifths  are  made  a  quarter  of  a  comma  too  But  with  Mr.  Bosanquet*s  fingerboard  (App.  ^ 

flat,  should  be  carried  out  through  twenty-six  XX.  sect.  F.  No.  8)  there  is  no  longer  any  need  to 

Fifths,  requiring  twenty-seven  tones  (namely,  limit  organs  to  12  notes  to  the  Octave.    When, 

7  natural,  7  sharp,  7  flat,  3  double  sharp,  and  however,  he  played  on  such  an  organ  before 

3  double  flat)  to  be  really  effective,  and  if  any  the  Musical  Association,  great  objection  was 

fewer  are  employed  no  attempt  should  be  made  taken    to  the  flatness  of   the  leading  note, 

to  modulate  into  keys  not  provided  with  proper  which  was  5^  cents  flatter  than  just,  as  musi- 

tones.    It  is  a  temperament  with  which  I  am  cians  are  accustomed  to  on6  which  is  12  cents 

practically  familiar.    It  is  harmonically  far  too  sharp.— Tratw^a^.] 
superior  to  the  equally  tempered,  and  is  even  {  Versuch  Uber  die  musikalische  Tempera- 

endurable  on  the  concertina,  which  used  to  be  tur^  Breslau,  1776. 

always  so  tuned,  but  having  fourteen  digitals,  §  Op,  cit.  p.  162.    The  following  works  of 

extends  from  ^  b  to  Dt .    The  twelve  digitals  these  two  authors  are  cited  by  Forkel :  Ncid- 

could  play  then  only  in  Bb,  F,  C,  G,  D,  and  hard   (Royal  Prussian  Band-conductor),  Die 

A  major,  and  in  G,  D,  and  A  minor,  which  of  teste    und  leickteste  Tempcratur  des   Mono- 

course  failed  to  satisfy  the  requirements  of  cJtordi  (the  best  and  easiest  temperament  of  the 
modulation.    Hence  players  sought  to  identify      monochord),  Jena,  1706  ;  Sectio  carwnis  har- 

Dff  ,  ^5  ,  J??jr ,  FJJ  ,  C«  «  ,  «a  J  with  E\),  B\>,  monici,  KSnigsberg,  1724.     Werck7n€ister  (or- 

F.  G,  />,  A  on  the  one  hand,  and  I>b,  Gb,  Cb,  ganist at  Quedlinburg,  bom  1645),  Miisikalische 
Ftfj  Bbby  Kb  by  Abb  with  CJJ,  FU  ,  D,  JC,  A,       Tempcratur,  Frankfurt  and  Li'ipziq,  i69i. 


Digitized  by 


v^oagle 


322  DISADVANTAGES  OF   TEMPERED  INTONATION,      part  m. 

work  of  great  autliority  iii  its  day  *  on  the  true  art  of  playing  the  clavier,'  requires 
this  instrument  to  be  always  tuned  in  the  equal  temperament.* 

The  old  attempts  to  introduce  more  than  12  degrees  into  the  scale  have  led 
to  nothing  practical,  because  they  did  not  start  from  any  right  principle.  They 
always  attached  themselves  to  the  Greek  system  of  Pythagoras,  and  imagined  only 
(hat  it  was  necessary  to  make  a  difference  between  dfi  and  d\}f  or  between  /$  and 
g\}y  and  so  on.  But  that  is  not  by  any  means  sufficient,  and  is  not  even  always 
correct.  According  to  our  system  of  notation  we  may  identify  c,Jjl  with  d\},  but 
we  must  distinguish  the  cfjj^  found  from  the  relation  of  Fifths,  from  the  c,j|!  found 
from  the  relation  of  Thirds.f  Hence  the  attempts  to  construct  instruments  with 
complex  arrangements  of  manuals  and  digitals,  have  led  to  no  result,  which  was 
at  all  commensurate  with  the  trouble  bestowed  upon  them,  and  the  increased 
difficulties  of  fingering  which  they  occasioned.  The  only  instrument  of  the  kind 
f  which  is  still  used  is  the  pedal  harp  a  double  mouvement,  on  which  tlie  intonation 
can  be  changed  by  the  foot. 

Not  only  habitual  use,  and  the  absence  of  any  power  to  compare  its  effects 
with  those  of  just  intonation,  but  some  other  circumstances  are  &vourable  to  equal 
temperament. 

First,  it  should  be  observed  that  the  disturbances  due  to  beats  in  the  tempered 
scale,  are  the  less  observable  the  swifter  the  motion  and  the  shorter  the  duration  of 
the  single  notes.  When  the  note  is  so  short  that  but  very  few  beats  can  possibly 
occur  while  it  lasts,  the  ear  has  no  time  to  remark  their  presence.  The  beats  pro- 
duced by  a  tempered  triad  are  the  following : 

I.  Beats  of  the  tempered  Fifth.  Suppose  we  take  the  number  of  vibrations  of 
o'  to  be  264,  the  tempered  Fifth  c''±.g'  would  produce  9  beats  in  10  seconds,  partly 
by  the  upper  partials,  and  partly  by  the  combinational  tones.  These  beats  are 
always  quite  audible. 
^  2.  Beats  of  the  two  first  combinational  tones  of  c'+6'  and  ^— ^'  in  tempered 
intonation  ;  5^  in  the  second.  These  are  plainly  audible  in  all  qualities  of  tones, 
if  the  tones  themselves  are  not  too  weak. 

3.  Beats  of  the  major  Third  c'+^'  alone,  10^  in  the  second,  which,  however, 
are  not  plainly  audible  unless  the  quahties  of  tone  employed  have  high  upper 
partials. 

4.  Beats  of  the  minor  Third  e'— ^',  18  in  the  second,  mostly  much  weaker  than 
those  of  the  major  Third,  and  also  heard  only  in  qualities  of  tone  having  high 
upper  partials. 

All  these  beats  occur  twice  as  fast  when  the  chord  lies  an  Octave  higher,  and 
half  as  fast  when  it  occurs  an  Octave  lower. 

Of  these  beats,  the  first,  arising  from  the  tempered  Fifths,  have  the  least 
injurious  influence  on  the  harmoniousness  of  the  chord.  They  are  so  slow  that 
they  can  be  heard  only  for  very  slow  notes  in  the  middle  parts  of  the  scale,  and 
^  then  they  produce  a  slow  undulation  of  the  chord  which  may  occasionally  have  a 
good  effect.  Beats  of  the  second  kind  are  most  striking  for  the  softer  quality  of 
tone.  In  an  Allegro,  of  four  crotchets  in  a  bar,  two  bars  occupy  about  three 
seconds.  If,  then,  the  tempered  chord  c'+e'— gr'  is  played  on  a  crotchet  in  this 
bar,  2^  of  these  beats  will  be  heard,  so  that  if  the  tone  begins  soft,  it  will  swell, 
decrease,  swell  again,  decrease  and  then  finish.  It  would  be  certainly  worse  if 
this  chord  were  played  an  Octave  or  two  higher,  so  that  ^\  or  8^  beats  could  be 
heard,  because  these  could  not  fail  to  strike  the  ear  as  a  marked  rouglmess. 

For  the  same  reason  the  beats  of  the  third  and  fourth  kinds,  arising  from  the 
Thirds,  which  are  clearly  audible  on  harsher  qualities  of  tone  (as  on  the  har- 
jnonium),  are  also  decidedly  disturbing  in  the  middle  positions,  even  in  quick  time» 

*   [Equal  temperament  was  not  commer-      perament,  pp.  27^32. — Translator.] 
cially  established  in  England  till  i84i>i846.  f  V^^^^  is*  ct  found  from  c±g±d^a±e 

See  App.  XX.  sect.  N.  No.  5.  With  regard  to  ±b±fZ  ±cff,  from  c,9  found  from  first  the 
both  Sebastian  and  Emanuel  Bach's  relation  to  major  Third  c  +  e^  and  then  the  Fifths  ^|  ± 
it,  see  Bosanquel's  Musical  Intervals  aitd  Tern-      6,  ±/,5  ±c,J  .-  Translator.] 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


CHAP.  XVI.      DISADVANTAGES  OF  TEMPERED  INTONATION.  323 

and  essentially  injure  the  calmness  of  the  triad,  because  they  are  twice  and  thrice 
as  fast  as  the  others.  It  is  only  in  soft  qualities  of  tone  that  they  are  but  little 
observed,  or,  when  observed,  are  so  covered  by  stronger  unbroken  tones  as  to  be 
very  slightly  marked. 

Hence  in  rapid  passages,  with  a  soft  quality  and  moderate  intensity  of  tone,  the 
evils  of  tempered  intonation  are  but  little  apparent.  Now,  almost  all  instrumental 
music  is  designed  for  rapid  movement,  and  this  forms  its  essential  advantage  over 
vocal  music.  We  might,  indeed,  raise  the  question  whether  instrumental  music 
had  not  rather  been  forced  into  rapidity  of  movement  by  this  very  tempered  into- 
nation, which  did  not  allow  us  to  feel  the  full  harmoniousness  of  slow  chords  to  the 
8ame  extent  as  is  possible  from  well-trained  singers,  and  that  instruments  had 
consequently  been  forced  to  renounce  this  branch  of  music. 

Tempered  intonation  was  first  and  especially  developed  on  the  pianoforte,  and 
hence  gradually  transferred  to  other  instruments.  Now,  on  the  pianoforte  circum-  f 
stances  really  f&vour  the  concealment  of  the  imperfections  due  to  the  temperament. 
The  tones  of  a  pianoforte  are  very  loud  only  at  the  moment  of  striking,  and  their 
loudness  rapidly  diminishes.  This,  as  I  have  already  had  occasion  to  mention, 
causes  their  combinational  tones  to  be  audible  at  the  first  moment  only,  and  hence 
makes  them  very  difficult  to  hear.  Beats  from  that  source  must  therefore  be  left 
out  of  consideration.  The  beats  which  depend  on  the  upper  partials  have  been 
eliminated  in  modern  pianofortes  (especially  in  the  higher  Octaves,  where  they 
would  have  done  most  harm),  owing  to  the  mode  in  which  upper  partials  are 
greatly  weakened  and  the  quality  of  tone  much  softened  by  regulating  the  striking 
place,  as  I  have  explained  in  Chap.  V.  (p.  776).  Hence  on  a  pianoforte  the  defi- 
ciencies of  the  intonation  are  less  marked  than  on  any  mstrument  with  sustained 
tones,  and  yet  are  not  quite  absent.  When  I  go  from  my  justly-intoned  harT- 
monium  to  a  grand  pianoforte,  every  note  of  the  latter  sounds  false  and  disturbing, 
especially  when  I  strike  isolated  successions  of  chords.  In  rapid  melodic  figures  f 
and  passages,  and  in  arpeggio  chords,  the  effect  is  less  disagreeable.  Hence  older 
musicians  especially  recommended  the  equal  temperament  for  the  pianoforte  alone. 
Matheson,  in  doing  so,  acknowledges  that  for  organs  Silberman*s  unequal  tempera- 
ment,  in  which  the  usual  keys  were  kept  pure,*  is  more  advantageous.  Emanuel 
Bach  says  that  a  correctly  tuned  pianoforte  has  the  most  perfect  intonation  of  all 
instruments^  which  in  the  above  sense  is  correct.  The  great  diffusion  and  conve- 
nience of  pianofortes  made  it  subsequently  the  chief  instrument  for  the  study  of 
music  and  its  intonation  the  pattern  for  that  of  all  other  instruments. 

On  the  other  hand,  for  the  harsher  stops  on  the  organ,  as  the  mixture  and  reed 
stops,  the  deficiencies  of  equal  temperament  are  extremely  striking.  It  is  con- 
sidered inevitable  that  when  the  mixture  stops  are  played  with  full  chords  an  aw  ful 
din  (hollenldrm)  must  ensue,  and  organists  have  submitted  to  their  fate.  Now  this 
is  mainly  due  to  equal  temperament,  because  if  the  Fifths  and  Thirds  in  the  pipe^ 
for  each  digital  of  the  mixture  stops  were  not  tuned  justly,  every  single  note  would  ^ 
produce  beats  by  itself.  But  when  the  Fifths  and  Thirds  between  the  notes 
belonging  to  the  different  digitals  are  tuned  in  equal  temperament,  every  chord 
famishes  at  once  tempered  and  just  Fifths  and  Thirds,  and  the  result  is  a  restless 
blurred  confusion  of  sounds.  And  yet  it  is  precisely  on  the  organ  it  would  be  so 
easy  by  a  few  stops  to  regulate  the  action  for  each  key  so  as  to  produce  harmonious 
chords.t 

Whoever  has  heard  the  difference  between  justly-intoned  and  tempered  chords, 
can  feel  no  doubt  that  it  would  be  the  greatest  possible  gain  for  a  large  organ  to 
omit  half  its  stops,  which  are  mostly  mere  toys,  and  double  the  number  of  tones 

*  [Probably  this  was  the  xneantone  tern-  1850  there  is  a  description  of  an  organ  by 
penunent  explained  on  p.  32 1,  note  t.— Trans-  Poole,  which  is  tuned  justly  for  all  keys 
lalor.]  by  means  of  stops.    fSee  App.  XVIII.  second 

t  From  Zamminer'B  book  (p.  140),  I  see  that  paragraph,  and  App.  XX.  sect.  F.  No.  7,  where 
in  Silliman's  American  Journal  of  Science  for      Poole's  new  keyboard  without  stops  is  figured.] 

Digitized  by^QK^QlC 


324  DISADVANTAGES   OF  TEMPERED   INTONATION.       part  iil 

in  the  Octave,  so  as  to  be  able,  by  means  of  suitable  stops,  to  play  correctly  in  all 
keys.* 

The  case  is  the  same  for  the  harmonium  as  for  these  stops  on  the  organ.  Its 
powerful  false  combinational  tones  and  its  gritty  trembling  chords,  both  due  U> 
tempered  intonation,  are  certainly  the  reason  why  many  musicians  pronounce  this 
instrument  to  be  out  of  tune,  and  dismiss  it  at  once  as  too  trying  to  the  nerves. 

Orchestral  instruments  can  generally  alter  their  pitch  slightly.  Bowed  instru- 
ments are  perfectly  unfettered  as  to  intonation  ;  wind  instruments  caji  be  made  a 
little  sharper  or  flatter  by  blowing  with  more  or  less  force.  They  are,  indeed,  all 
adapted  for  equal  temperament,  but  good  players  have  the  means  of  indulging  tlie 
ear  to  some  extent.  Hence,  passages  in  Thirds  for  wind  instruments,  when 
executed  by  indifferent  players,  often  sound  desperately  false  (verzweifelt  falsch), 
whereas  good  performers,  with  delicate  ears,  make  them  sound  perfectly  well. 

^  The  bowed  instruments  are  peculiar.  From  the  first  they  have  retained  their 
tuning  in  perfect  Fifths.  The  violins  themselves  have  the  perfect  Fifths,  G±D 
±i4±jB.  The  tenor  and  violoncello  give  the  Fifth  C:hG  in  addition.  Now, 
every  scale  has  its  own  peculiar  fingering,  and  hence  every  pupil  could  be  easOy 
practised  in  playing  each  scale  in  its  proper  intonation,  and  then,  of  course,  tones 
of  the  same  name  but  in  different  keys  could  not  be  rtopped  in  the  same  way,  and 
even  the  major  Third  of  tlie  major  scale  of  C,  when  the  C  of  the  tenor  is  taken 
as  the  tonic,  must  not  be  played  on  the  E  string  of  the  violin,  because  this  gi^-es 
E  and  not  ^j.  Nevertheless,  tlie  modem  school  of  violin -playing  since  the  time 
of  Spohr,  aims  especially  at  producmg  equally-tempered  intonation,  although  this 
caimot  be  completely  attained,  owing  to  the  perfect  Fifths  of  the  open  strings.  At 
any  rate,  the  acknowledged  intention  of  present  violin-players  is  to  produce  only 
1 2  degrees  in  the  Octave.  The  sole  exception  which  they  allow  is  for  double-stop 
passages,  in  which  the  notes  have  to  be  somewhat  differently  stopped  from  what 

fjf  they  are  when  played  alone.  But  this  exception  is  decisive.  In  double-stop 
passages  the  individual  player  feels  himself  responsible  for  the  harmoniousness  <tf 
the  interval,  and  it  lies  completely  within  his  power  to  make  it  good  or  bad.  Any 
violin-player  will  easily  be  able  to  verify  the  following  &ct.  Tune  the  strings  of 
the  violin  in  the  perfect  Fifths  G±D±.A±,E,  and  find  where  the  finger  mast  be 
pressed  on  the  A  string  to  produce  the  B,  which  will  give  a  perfect  Fourth  B...E, 
Now,  let  him,  without  moving  his  finger,  strike  this  same  B  together  with  the 
open  D  string.  The  interval  D...B  would,  according  to  the  usual  view,  be  a 
major  Sixth,  but  it  would  be  a  Pythagorean  one  [of  906  cents].  In  order  to 
obtain  iiie  consonant  Sixth  D...B1  [of  884  cents],  the  finger  would  have  to  be 
drawn  back  for  about  ij  Paris  lines  (nearly  ^%  inch),  a  distance  quite  appreciable 
in  stoppings,  and  sufficient  to  alter  the  pitch  and  the  beauty  of  the  consonance 
most  perceptibly. 

But  it  is  clear  that  if  individual  players  feel  themselves  obliged  to  distinguish 

^  the  different  values  of  the  notes  in  the  different  consonances,  there  is  no  reason 
why  the  bad  Thirds  of  the  Pythagorean  series  of  Fifths  should  be  retained  in 
quartett  playing.  Chords  of  several  parts,  executed  by  several  performers  in 
a  quartett,  often  sound  very  ill,  even  when  each  single  one  of  these  performers 
can  perform  solo  pieces  very  well  and  pleasantly ;  and,  on  the  otlier  hand,  when 
quartetts  are  played  by  finely-cultivated  artists,  it  is  impossible  to  detect  any  false 
consonances.  To  my  mind  the  only  assignable  reason  for  these  results  is  that 
practised  violinists  with  a  delicate  sense  of  harmony,  know  how  to  stop  the  tones 
they  want  to  hear,  and  hence  do  not  submit  to  the  rules  of  an  imperfect  school. 

*  [That  is,  as  correctly  as  on  the  Author's  more  than  three-quarters  instead  of  only  on€- 

justly-intoned  harmoniam,  but  that  is  far  too  half  of  the  stops.    And  then  musicians  would 

deficient  in  power  of  modulation  into  minor  have  to  learn  how  to  use  a  practically  just 

keys,  to  make  it  worth  while  to  construct  it  scale,  and  how  to  adapt  tempered  music  to  it, 

on  a  great  orgali.     Nothing  short  of  the   53  both  of  which  present  considerable  difiBiculties. 

division  of  the  Octave  (p.  328c)  would  suffice,  It  is,  the  refore,  safe  to  say  that  nothing  of  the 

and   this  would   necessitate   the  omission   of  kind  will  be  done. — Translator. ^^ 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


CHAP.  XVI.      DISADVANTAGES  OF  TEMPERED  INTONATION.  323 

That  performers  of  the  highest  rank  do  really  play  in  just  intonation,  has  been 
directly  proved  by  the  very  interesting  and  exact  results  of  Delezenne.*  This 
observer  determined  the  individual  notes  of  the  major  scale,  as  it  was  played  by 
distinguished  violinists  and  violoncellists,  by  means  of  an  accurately  gauged  string, 
and  found  that  these  players  produced  correctly  perfect  Thirds  and  Sixths,  and 
neither  equally  tempered  nor  Pythagorean  Thirds  or  Sixths.  I  was  fortunate 
enough  to  have  an  opportunity  of  making  similar  observations  by  means  of  my 
liarmonium  on  Herr  Joachim.  He  tuned  his  violin  exactly  with  the  g±d±a±e 
of  my  instrument.  I  then  requested  him  to  play  the  scale,  and  immediately  he 
had  played  the  Third  or  Sixth,  I  gave  the  corresponding  note  on  the  harmonium. 
By  means  of  beats  it  was  easy  to  determine  that  this  distinguished  musician  used 
bi  and  not  b  as  the  major  Third  to  g,  and  Sy  not  e  as  the  Sixth.f  But  if  the  best 
players  who  are  thoroughly  acquainted  with  what  they  are  playing  are  able 
to  overcome  the  defects  of  their  school  and  of  the  tempered  system,  it  would  ^ 
certainly  wonderfully  smooth  the  path  of  performers  of  the  second  order,  in  their 
attempts  to  attain  a  perfect  ensemble^  if  they  had  been  accustomed  from  the  first 
to  play  the  scales  by  natural  intervals.  The  greater  trouble  attending  the  first 
attempts  would  be  amply  repaid  by  the  result  when  the  ear  has  once  become 
accustomed  to  hear  perfect  consonances.  It  is  really  much  easier  to  apprehend 
the  differences  between  notes  of  the  same  name  in  just  intonation  than  people 
usually  imagine,  when  the  ear  has  once  become  accustomed  to  the  effect  of  just 
consonances.  A  confusion  between  a^  and  a  in  a  consonant  chord  on  my  har- 
monium strikes  me  with  the  same  readiness  and  certainty  as  a  confusion  between 
A  and  A\}  on  b^  pianoforte.it 

I  am,  however,  too  httle  acquainted  with  the  technicalities  of  violin-playing, 
to  attempt  making  any  proposals  for  a  definite  regulation  of  the  tonal  system  of 
bowed  instruments.  This  must  be  left  to  masters  of  this  instrument  who  at  the 
same  time  possess  the  powers  of  a  composer.  Such  men  will  readily  convince  % 
themselves  by  the  testimony  of  their  ears,  that  the  facts  here  adduced  are  correct, 
and  perceive  that,  far  from  being  useless  mathematical  speculations,  they  are 
practical  questions  of  very  great  importance. 

The  case  is  precisely  similar  for  our  present  singers.  For  singing,  intonation 
is  perfectly  free,  whereas  on  bowed  instruments,  the  five  tones  of  the  open  strings 
at  least  have  an  unalterable  pitch.  In  singing  the  pitch  can  be  made  most  easily 
and  perfectly  to  follow  the  wishes  of  a  fine  musical  ear.  Hence  all  music  began 
with  singing  ;§  and  singing  will  always  remain  the  true  and  natural  school  of  all 
music.  The  only  intervals  which  singers  can  strike  with  certainty  and  perfection, 
are  such  as  they  can  comprehend  with  certainty  and  perfection,  and  what  the 
singer  easily  and  naturally  sings  the  hearer  will  also  easily  and  naturally  under- 
Btand.** 

*  Recueil  des  Travaux  de  la  SodiU  des  when  isolated  from  the  rest  of  the  scale,  I 

Sciences f  de  V Agriculture,  et  des  Arts  de  Lille,  find  it  difficult  to  distinguish  between  the  just  ^ 

1826  et  premier  semestre  1827  ;  Mimoire  sur  and  the  Pythagorean  major  Third.    But  when 

les  Valeurs  nuirUrique  des  Notes  de  la  Oatnme,  I  play  on  my  harmonium  the  complete  melody 

par  M.  Delezenne.     [See  especially  pp.  55-6.]  of  some  well-known  air  without  harmonies  the 

For  observations  on    corresponding    circum-  Pythagorean  Third  always  feels  tome  strained, 

stances  in  singing,  see  Appendix  XVIII.  the  perfect  Third  calm  and  soft.    It  is  only  in 

t  Messrs.  Comu  and  Mercadier  have  indeed  the  leading  note,  perhaps,  that  the  sharper 

published  contradictory  observations.  {Comptes  Third  is  more  expressive.      [See  App.   XX. 

liendus  de  VAcad.  des  Sc,  de  Paris,  8  et  «2  sect.  G.  arts.  6  and  7,  for  the  results  of  later 

F^vrier,  1869.)     They  let  a  musician  play  the  experiments  by  Messrs.  Comu  and  Mercadier. 

Third  of  a  major  chord  firet  in  melodic  sue-  — Translator.] 

cession,  and  then  in  harmonious  consonance.  X  U^  ^  consonant  chord  the  difference  is 

In  the  latter  case  it  was  always  4:5.    But  striking,    melodically  not  so.      An  eminent 

in  melodic  succession  the  performer  selected  teacher  of  singing  could  only  by  great  atten- 

a  somewhat  sharper  Third.    I  am  bound  to  tion  tell  the  difference  when  I  alternated  a„  a 

reply,  that  in  melodic  succession  the  major  and  d„  c2  in  the  major  scale  of  C. — Translator.] 
Third  is  not  a  very  characteristically  deter-  §  [It  must  not  be  forgotten,  however,  that 

mined  interval,  and  that  all  living  musicians  the  voice  was  the  only  musical  instrument  at 

have  been  accustomed  to  sharp  Thirds  on  the  first  knovfn.— Translator.] 
pianoforte.    In  the  simple  succession  c  +  e-y,  ♦♦  [That  this  must  also  apply  to  non-har* 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


326  DISADVANTAGES  OF  TEMPEEED  INTONATION.      PAKTin. 

Down  to  the  seventeenth  century  singers  were  practised  by  the  monocbord,  for 
which  Zarlino  in  the  middle  of  the  sixteentli  century  reintroduced  the  correct 
natural  intonation.  Singers  were  then  practised  with  a  degree  of  care  of  which 
we  have  at  present  no  conception.  We  can  even  now  see  from  the  Italian  music 
of  the  fifteenth  and  sixteenth  centuries  that  they  were  calculated  for  most  periiBct 
intonation  of  the  chords,  and  that  their  whole  effect  is  destroyed  as  soon  as  this 
intonation  is  executed  with  insufficient  precision. 

But  it  is  impossible  not  to  acknowledge  that  at  the  present  day  few  even  of  our 
opera  singers  are  able  to  execute  a  httle  piece  for  several  voices,  when  either 
totally  unaccompanied,  or  at  most  accompanied  by  occasional  chords,  (as,  for 
example,  tlie  trio  for  the  three  masks,  Protegga  it  giusto  cielOy  from  the  finale  to 
the  first  act  of  Mozart *s  Don  Giovanni,)  in  a  manner  suited  to  give  the  hearer  a 
full  enjoyment  of  its  perfect  harmony.  The  chords  almost  always  sound  a  little 
%  sharp  or  uncertain,  so  that  they  disturb  a  musical  hearer.  But  where  are  our 
singers  to  learn  just  intonation  and  make  their  ears  sensitive  for  perfect  chords  ? 
They  are  from  the  first  taught  to  sing  to  the  equally-tempered  pianoforte.  If  a 
major  chord  is  struck  as  an  accompaniment,  they  may  sing  a  perfect  consonance 
with  its  root,  its  Fifth,  or  its  Third.  This  gives  them  about  the  fifth  part  of  a 
Semitone  for  their  voices  to  choose  from  without  decidedly  singing  out  of  harmony, 
and  even  if  tliey  sing  a  little  sharper  than  consonance  witli  the  sharp  Third 
requires,  or  a  little  flatter  than  consonance  with  the  flat  Fifth  requires,  the 
harmoniousness  of  the  chord  will  not  be  really  much  more  damaged.  The  singer 
who  practises  to  a  tempered  instrument  has  no  principle  at  all  for  exactly  and 
certainly  determining  the  pitch  of  his  voice.* 

On  tlie  other  hand,  we  often  hear  four  musical  amateurs  who  have  practised 
much  together,  singing  quartetts  in  perfectly  just  intonation.  Indeed,  my  own 
experience  leads  me  almost  to  affirm  that  quartetts  are  more  frequently  heard 
5  with  just  intonation  when  smig  by  young  men  who  scarcely  sing  anything  else, 
and  often  and  regularly  practise  them,  than  when  sung  by  instructed  solo  singers 
who  are  accustomed  to  the  accompaniment  of  the  pianoforte  or  the  orchestra.  But 
correct  intonation  in  singing  is  so  far  above  all  others  tlie  first  condition  of  beauty, 
that  a  song  when  sung  in  correct  intonation  even  by  a  weak  and  unpractised  voice 
always  sounds  agreeable,  whereas  the  richest  and  most  practised  voice  offends  the 
hearer  when  it  sings  false,  or  sharpens. 

The  case  is  the  same  as  for  bowed  instruments.  The  instruction  of  our  present 
singers  by  means  of  tempered  instruments  is  unsatisfactory,  but  those  who  possess 
good  musical  talents  are  ultimately  able  by  their  own  practice  to  strike  out  the 
right  path  for  themselves,  and  overcome  the  error  of  their  original  instruction. 
They  even  succeed  the  earlier,  perhaps,  the  sooner  they  quit  school,  although,  of 
course,  I  do  not  mean  to  deny  that  fluency  in  singing,  and  the  disuse  of  all  kinds 
of  bad  ways  can  only  be  acquired  in  school. 
f  It  is  clearly  not  necessary  to  temper  the  instruments  to  which  the  singer 
practises.  A  single  key  suffices  for  these  exercises,  and  that  can  be  correctly 
tuned.  We  do  not  require  to  use  the  same  piano  for  the  teaching  to  sing  and  for 
playing  sonatas.  Of  course  it  would  be  better  to  practise  the  singer  to  a  justly* 
intoned  organ  or  harmonium  in  which  by  means  of  two  manuals  all  keys  may  be 
used.t  Sustained  tones  are  preferable  as  an  accompaniment  because  the  singer 
himself  can  immediately  hear  the  beats  between  the  instrument  and  his  voice 
when  he  alters  the  pitch  slightly.     Draw  his  attention  to  these  beats,  and  he  will 

monic  scales  is  evident  from  the  fact  that  wo  aid  not  suit.  Still  the  Harmonioal,  or,  for 
music  has  existed  for  thousands  of  years,  but  modulating  purposes,  the  just  harmoniom  or 
harmonic  scales  have  been  in  use  only  a  few  just  concertina,  may  prove  of  service.  Other- 
centuries,  and  are  far  from  being  even  yet  wise  special  instruments  must  be  used,  as  Mr. 
univeraal.—  Translator.]  Colin  Brown's  Voice-Harmonium.  The  Tonic 
♦  See  Appendix  XVIII.  So^.faists  teach  without  any  accompaniment, 
t  ;  Voices  differ  so  much  that  the  bame  not  even  that  of  the  teacher's  voic^e,  but 
pitch   for  the   tonic,  that  is  the  same  key,  rapidly  introduce  part  music— IVaHfiiotor." 


Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


CHA?.  XVI.        DISADVANTAGES  OF  TEMPERED  INTONATION.         327 

then  have  a  means  of  cbeckiiig  his  own  voice  in  the  most  decisive  manner.  This 
is  very  easy  on  my  justly-intoned  harmonium,  as  I  know  by  experience.  It  is 
only  after  the  singer  has  learned  to  hear  every  slight  deviation  from  correctness 
announced  by  a  striking  incident,  that  it  becomes  possible  for  him  to  regulate  the 
motions  of  his  larynx  and  the  tension  of  his  vocal  chords  with  sufficient  delicacy 
to  produce  the  tone  which  his  ear  demands.  When  we  require  a  delicate  use  of 
the  muscles  of  any  part  of  the  human  body,  as,  in  this  case,  of  the  larynx,  there 
must  be  some  sure  means  of  ascertaining  whether  success  has  been  attained.  Now 
the  presence  or  absence  of  beats  gives  such  a  means  of  detecting  success  or  failure 
when  a  voice  is  accompanied  by  sustained  chords  in  just  intonation.  But  tempered 
chords  which  produce  beats  of  their  own  are  necessarily  quite  unsuited  for  such  a 
purpose. 

Finally,  we  cannot,  I  think,  fail  to  recognise  the  influence  of  tempered  intona- 
tion upon  the  style  of  composition.  The  iirst  effect  of  this  influence  was  favourable,  f' 
It  allowed  composers  as  well  as  players  to  move  freely  and  easily  into  all  keys,  and 
thus  opened  up  a  new  weaAth  of  modulation.  On  the  other  hand,  we  hkewise  cannot 
fail  to  recognise  that  the  alteration  of  intonation  also  compelled  composers  to  have 
recourse  to  some  such  wealth  of  modulation.  For  when  the  intonation  of  consonant 
chords  ceased  to  be  perfect,  and  the  differences  between  their  various  inversions 
and  positions  were,  as  a  consequence,  nearly  obhterated,  it  was  necessary  to  use 
more  powerful  means,  to  have  recourse  to  a  frequent  employment  of  harsh  disso- 
nances, and  to  endeavour  by  less  usual  modulations  to  replace  the  characteristic 
expression,  which  the  harmonies  proper  to  the  key  itself  had  ceased  to  possess. 
Hence  in  many  modem  compositions  dissonant  chords  of  the  dominant  Seventh 
form  the  majority,  and  consonant  chords  the  minority,  yet  no  one  can  doubt  that 
this  is  the  reverse  of  what  ought  to  be  the  case ;  and  continual  bold  modulational 
leaps  threaten  entirely  to  destroy  the  feehng  for  tonahty.  These  are  unpleasant 
symptoms  for  the  further  development  of  art.  The  mechanism  of  instruments  ^ 
and  attention  to  their  convenience,  threaten  to  lord  it  over  the  natural  require- 
ments of  the  ear,  and  to  destroy  once  more  the  principle  upon  which  modem 
musical  art  is  founded,  the  steady  predominance  of  the  tonic  tone  and  tonic  chord. 
Among  our  great  composers,  Mozart  and  Beethoven  were  yet  at  the  commence- 
ment of  the  reign  of  equal  temperament.  Mozart  had  still  an  opportunity  of 
making  extensive  studies  in  the  composition  of  song.  He  is  master  of  the  sweetest 
possible  harmoniousness,  where  he  desires  it,  but  he  is  almost  the  last  of  such 
masters.  Beethoven  eagerly  and  boldly  seized  the  wealth  offered  by  instrumental 
music,  and  in  his  powerful  hands  it  became  the  appropriate  and  ready  tool  for 
producing  effects  which  none  had  hitherto  attempted.  But  he  used  the  human 
voice  as  a  mere  handmaid,  and  consequently  she  has  also  not  lavished  on  him  the 
highest  magic  of  her  beauty. 

And  after  all,  I  do  not  know  that  it  was  so  necessary  to  sacrifice  correctness  of 
intonation  to  the  convenience  of  musical  instruments.  As  soon  as  violinists  have  f 
resolved  to  play  every  scale  in  just  intonation,  which  can  scarcely  occasion  any 
difliculty,  the  other  orchestral  instruments  will  have  to  suit  themselves  to  the 
correcter  intonation  of  the  violins.  Horns  and  trumpets  have  already  naturally 
just  intonation.* 

Moreover,  we  must  observe  that  when  just  intonation  is  made  the  groundwork 
of  modulations,  even  comparatively  simple  modulational  excursions  will  occasion 
enharmonic  confusions  (amounting  to  a  comma)  which  do  not  appear  as  such  in 
the  tempered  system.f 

To  me  it  seems  necessary  that  the  new  tonic  into  which  we  modulate  should 

'*'   [Beferring  to  this  passage,  Mr.  Blaikley  in   practice/     See   the  whole  of   this  paper, 

says  (Proceediyigs  of  the  Musical  Association^  and  the  discussion  on  it,  and  also  see  supra, 

vol.  iv.  p.  56) :  '  This  must  be  taken  as  being  pp.  97^,  996,  notes.— Translator,] 

particularly  and  not  generally  true,   that   is,  t   [See  p.  324«{,note  '*',  and  p.  340c,  note  *• 

though  the  ideal  instrument  has  such  charac-  —Translator.] 
teristics,  this  ideal  is  not  necessarily  attained 


Digitized  by 


Google 


3f8  RULES  OF  MODULATION.  .  pawt  iii. 

be  related  to  the  tonic  in  which  we  are  playing ;  the  nearer  the  relationship,  the 
more  striking  the  transition.  Again,  it  is  not  advisable  to  remain  long  in  a  key 
which  is  not  related  to  the  principal  tonic  of  the  piece.  With  these  principles  the 
rules  for  modulation  usually  given  coincide.  The  easiest  and  most  usual  tran- 
sitions are  into  the  key  of  the  dominant  or  subdominant,  these  tones  being,  as  is 
well  known,  the  nearest  relations  of  the  first  tonio>  Henoe  if  the  original  key  is  C, 
we  can  pass  immediately  into  G  major,  and  thus  change  the  tones  F  and  Ai  of  the 
scale  of  C  major  into  F^jf^  and  A.  Or  we  can  pass  into  F  major  by  exchanging  i?, 
and  D  for  B\}  and  Di.  After  this  step  has  been  made,  the  music  will  often  pass 
into  a  key  with  a  tonic  related  to  C  in  the  second  degree  only,  as  from  GtoDor 
from  F  to  B\}.  By  proceeding  in  this  way  we  should  come  to  keys  as  A  and  ^t>, 
of  which  the  relation  to  the  original  tonic  C  would  be  very  obscure  and  in  which 
it  would  certainly  not  be  advisable  to  remain  long  for  fear  of  too  much  weakening 
f  the  feeling  for  the  original  tonic. 

Again,  we  may  also  modulate  from  the  principal  tonic  C  to  its  Thirds  and 
Sixths,  to  E^  and  A^  or  E^\}  and  A^\}.  Li  tempered  intonation  these  changes 
seem  to  be  the  same  as  from  O  and  D  to  A  and  E^  or  from  F  and  B\}  to  E\}  and 
A\},  But  they  differ  in  the  pitch,  as  shewn  by  the  different  marks  A  and  ^i,  &c. 
In  the  tempered  intonation  it  seems  allowable  to  go  by  a  Sixth  from  c  to  the  key  a, 
and  then  by  Fifths  back,  to  d,  g,  and  then  c  again.  But  in  reality  we  thus  reach 
a  d  fferent  c  from  that  with  which  we  began.  By  such  a  transition,  which  is 
certainly  not  quite  natural,  we  should  be  obliged  to  make  an  enharmonic  exchange 
[alteration  of  pitch  by  a  comma],  and  this  would  be  best  done  while  in  the  key  of  <f, 
since  both  d  and  dy  are  related  to  c  in  the  second  degree.  In  the  comphcated 
modulations  of  modem  composers  such  enharmonic  changes  will  of  course  have 
to  be  often  made.  A  cultivated  taste  will  have  to  judge  in  each  individual  case 
how  they  are  to  be  introduced,  but  it  will  be  probably  advisable  to  retain  the  roles 
f  already  mentioned,  and  to  choose  the  intonation  of  the  new  tonics  introduced  by 
modulation  m  such  a  manner  as  will  keep  them  as  closely  related  to  the  prin* 
cipal  tonic  as  possible.  Enharmonic  changes  are  least  observed  when  they  are 
made  immediately  before  or  after  strongly  dissonant  chords,  as  those  of  the 
diminished  Seventh.  Such  enharmonic  changes  of  pitch  are  already  sometimes 
clearly  and  intentionally  made  by  violinists,  and  where  they  are  suitable  even  pro* 
duce  a  very  good  effect.* 

t  If  we  desire  to  produce  a  scale  in  almost  precisely  just  intonation,  which  will 
allow  of  an  indefinite  power  of  modulation  without  having  recourse  to  enhar- 
monic changes,^  we  can  effect  our  purpose  by  the  division  of  the  Octave  into  53 
exactly  equal  parts,  as  was  long  ago  proposed  by  Mercator  [to  represent  Pytha- 
gorean intonation].  Mr.  E.  H.  M.  Bosanquet§  has  recently  provided  this  tem- 
perament, as  realised  on  an  harmonium,  with  a  symmetrically  arranged  finger- 
board. When  the  Octave  is  divided  into  53  equal  intervals  or  degrees,  31  such 
^  degrees  give  an  almost  perfect  Fifth,  the  error  of  which  is  only  ^\  of  the  error 
of  the  Fifth  of  the  usual  equal  temperament,  and  17  of  these  degrees  give  a  major 
Third,  of  which  the  error  in  defect  is  only  ^  of  the  above-named  error  of  the  Fifth 
in  equal  temperament.**     The  error  of  the  Fifth  in  this  system  must  be  considered 

*  See  examples  in  C.  E.  Naumann's  JSes^im-  in  Boom  Q  of  the  Scientific  Collections  ai  the 

mungen  der  Tonverhdltnisse  (Determinations  South  Kensington  Maseam]. 
ol  the  Tonal  Katios),  Leipzig,  1858,  pp.  48,  sqq.  **  On  converting  the  ratio  of  the  extent  of 

f  [From  here  to  the  end  of  the  chapter  the  interval  of  a  Fifth  to  that  of  an  Octave 

is  an  addition  to  the  4th  German  edition. —  (that  is  log.  1-5  .*  log.  2)  into  a  continued  frac- 

Translator.]  tion,  we  get  the  following  approximations  : 

t  [This    is    unfortunately  not  the    case  j^  --  y^  Fifths, 

when  translating  equally  tempered  music,  as  nearly  =  7  31  179  Octaves 

shewn  by  the  last  example.— Tratwia/or.]  \ 

§  An    Elementary   Treatise  on    Musical  And  by  a  similar  approximation 
Intervals  and    Temperament,  London,  Mac-  ^  28  59  major  Thirds- 

millan,  1875.    The  instrument  described  was  nearly -i  9  19  Octaves, 

exhibited  in  the  Scientific  Loan  Exhibition  at 
South  Kensington  [in  May  1876,  and  is  still  TAs  these  approximations  give  no  coneep- 

Digitized  by  VjOOQlC 


QHAP.  XVI. 


THE  CYCLE   OF  FIFTY-THREE. 


329 


as  quite  inappreciable,  that  of  the  major  Third  is  still  more  difficult  to  perceive  than 
that  of  the  equally  tempered  Fifth.*    In  these  degrees  the  major  scale  will  be 

C  D  E,  F  G  Ay  By  G 
degrees  o  9  17  22  31  39  48  53 
differences    9859895 

These  differences  of  9,  8,  5  correspond  to  the  major,  minor,  and  half  Tone  of 
the  just  scale.  Each  separate  degree  of  the  scale  corresponds  nearly  with  the 
interval  77  :  76  [=2 2 '6  cents]  and  is  therefore  extremely  little  greater  than  the 
comma  81  :  80  [=21*5  cents],  which  in  the  just  scale  gives  the  difference  between 
a  large  or  diatonic  Semitone  [16  :  i5=:ii2  cents]  and  a  small  Semitone  or  limma 
[256  :  243=90  cents] .  The  ear  cannot  distinguish  this  scale  from  the  juBt,t  and 
in  its  practical  applications  it  admits  of  unhmited  modulation  in  what  is  equal  to 
exact  intonation.  The  difference  between  our  c,  and  c,  or  our  c  and  c'  would  answer 
to  sharpening  by  one  degree.  Mr.  Bosanquet  therefore  employs  the  convenien'  IT 
signs  \c  for  c,  and  /c  for  c*,  Wc  for  c^,  &c.  These  signs,  \and  /,  he  also 
employs  before  notes  on  the  staff,  exactly  as  we  employ  jf  and  \},  The  fingerboard  is 
arranged  in  a  very  comprehensible  and  symmetrical  way  to  make  the  fingering  of 
all  scales  and  all  chords  the  same  in  all  keys.t  A  diagram  of  the  keyboard  will 
be  found  in  App.  XIX. § 

Perhaps  a  justification  is  needed  for  our  having  in  this  whole  theory  of  keys 
and  modulations,  identified  the  key  of  the  Octave  with  that  of  its  root,  while 


tion  of  the  extreme  closeness  with  which  the 
53  division,  if  accurately  taned,  woald  approxi- 


mate to  just  intonation,  I  annex  the  following 
table : 


Note 

Ju  ^t  cents 

53  divUion 
cents 

No.  of 
degrees 

c 

0 

0 

0 

c» 

21-506 

22-642 

I 

^» 

182*404 

181-132 

8 

D 

203910 

203-774 

9 

E^b 

3'5-64i 

316-981 

14 

E 

386-314 

384906 

17 

ip 

470781 

475-472 

21 

F 

498-045 

498-113 

22 

0 

701955 

701-887 

31 

At, 

813-687 

815-094 

36 

At 

884-359 

883019 

39 

'Bb 

968-^26 

973-585 

43 

Bb 

996091 

996-226 

44 

J5'b 

1017-597 

1018-868 

45 

^» 

1088-269 

1086-792 

48 

C 

1200000 

1200000 

53 

»'Db 

1304-955 

1313-208 

58 

1 

Hence  for  all  tertian  intervals  the  approxima- 
tion is  within  2  cents,  often  within  i  cent. 
The  septimal  comma  being  greater  than  I 
degree,  the  'Bb  is  too  sharp  by  5  cents. 
StiU  the  53  div.  chord  C  I  E,  Gl'Bb  ish 
great  improvement  on  the  just  C  :  E^  :  G  :  Bb. 
The  17^1  harmonic  is  8  cents  too  sharp,  but 
the  chord  of  the  diminished  Seventh  in  the 
53  div.  Ej  :  O  :  'Bb  :  ^^Bb  is  much  superior 
to  the  just  form  E^:  O:  Bb:  D'b,  though 
from  its  just  surroundings  inferior  in.  effect 
to  the  equally  tempered  E  :  O  :  Bb  I  Db.— 
Translator,] 

*  [Both,  however,  give  rise  to  beats  which 
are  of  great  importance  to  the  tuner.  See 
App.  XX.  sect.  G.  art.  20.— Translator.] 

t  [Melodically ;  but  harmonically,  at  least 
as  the  intervals  were  tuned  on  Mr.  Bosanquet's 
instrument,  there  was  a  decidedly  perceptible 
difference  to  an  ear,  accustomed  as  mine  was, 
to  listen  to  just  intonation.     Traimlator.] 


%  [Prof.  Helmholtz  adds,  *  after  a  plan  in- 
vented by  the  American,  Mr.  H.  W.  Poole.*  I  fl 
have  omitted  this  line  because,  although  Mr. 
Poolers  remarkable  fingerboard  (figured  in 
App.  XX.  sect.  F.  No.  6)  also  allows  of  playing 
with  the  same  fingering  in  all  keys,  it  was 
not  intended  for  the  53  division,  and  it  bears 
no  resemblance  to  that  of  Mr.  Bosanquet, 
who  has  the  exclusive  merit  of  inventing  and 
practically  carrying  out  his  extraordinary  *  gene- 
ralised keyboard,'  which  is  suitable  for  all  cycles 
(except  the  ordinary  one  of  12)  that  resolve  the 
tones  used  into  a  series  of  tempered  Fifths.  See 
App.  XX.  sect.  A.  art.  20  ^({.—Translator,] 

§  [In  App.  XX.  sect.  F.  No.  8, 1  have  added 
a  further  account  of  this  invention,  and  (ibii. 
No.  9)  a  notice  of  another  keyboard  for  a  reed 
instrument  called  the  Harmon^  also  using  the 
53  division,  invented  and  executed  by  Mr.  James 
Paul  White,  a  tuner,  of  Springfield,  Massachu- 
setts, U.S.  kmetioik. —Translator.] 

Digitized  by  VjOOQlC 


330  DISSONANT  INTERVALS.  pakt  in. 

we  have  distinguished  the  key  of  the  Twelfth.  lii  tlie  usual  school  of  musical 
theory,  the  meaning  of  the  sound  of  the  Octave  is  completely  identified  with  that 
of  its  root,  and  is  so  treated.  For  us,  on  the  other  hand,  the  Octave  is  only  the 
Tone  most  nearly  and  clearly  related  to  the  root;  but  its  relationship  is  the  same 
in  kind  as  that  of  the  Twelfth,  or  the  next  higher  major  Third  (Seventeenth)  to 
the  root. 

Now  we  have  shewn  in  p.  273a  that  in  the  particular  relation  of  the  forma- 
tion of  scales,  that  is  of  the  determination  of  the  key,  the  higher  Octave  introduces 
the  same  series  of  directly  related  tones  as  does  the  lower,  although  in  a  somewhat 
different  order  of  strength  of  relationship.  Only  throughout  the  formation  of  the 
lower  Octave  the  tones  of  the  major  scale  are  favoured,  and  in  the  formation  of 
the  upper  Octave  those  of  the  minor  scale  are  preferred,  but  not  to  the  exclusion 
of  those  of  other  scales. 

m  When  we  proceed  beyond  the  limits  of  the  first  Octave,  the  relationships  of 
tone  depending  on  the  six  first  partials  give  only  the  Tenth  and  Twelfth.  The 
other  steps  of  the  scales  have  then  to  be  filled  up  with  tones  related  in  the  second 
degree,  and,  among  these,  the  relations  of  the  Octave  must  have  the  preference, 
and  next  those  of  the  Twelfth.  H.nce  in  the  second  Octave  we  have  necessarily 
a  repetition  of  the  scale  of  the  first.  By  this  means,  in  the  formation  of  scales  an 
equivalence  of  Octaves  is  established,  without  any  necessity  for  assuming  a  speci- 
fically different  relation  of  similarity  between  them  and  the  root,  as  we  had  to  do 
for  the  other  consonances.  In  the  formation  of  consonant  intervals,  the  usual 
theory  of  music  also  considers  the  Octaves  as  equivalent  to  the  roots.  This  is 
within  certain  hmits  correct,  because  the  intervals  usually  considered  as  consonant, 
remain  consonant  when  one  of  their  tones  is  transposed  by  an  Octave,  or  at  least 
produce  intervals  which  lie  on  the  limits  of  consonance.  But  here  the  usual  rule 
of  the  school  really  gave  a  very  imperfect  expression  of  the  facts^  since,  as  we 

^  have  shewn  in  Chapters  X.,  XI.,  and  XII.,  the  degree  and  sequence  of  the  con- 
sonance are  really  materially  altered  by  these  changes,  and  composers  who  have 
outgrown  the  rules  of  the  school,  have  also  very  clearly  had  regard  to  tliese 
alterations. 


CHAPTER  XVII. 

OF  DISGOBDS. 

When  voices  move  foi-ward  melodically  in  part  music,  the  general  rule  is  that  they 
must  form  consonances  with  each  other.  For  it  is  only  as  long  as  they  are  con- 
sonant, that  there  is  an  uninterrupted  fusion  of  the  corresponding  auditory  seusa- 
tions.  As  soon  as  they  are  dissonant  the  individual  parts  mutually  disturb  each 
other,  and  each  is  a  hindrance  to  the  £ree  motion  of  the  other.  To  this  estlietic 
reason  must  be  added  the  purely  physical  consideration,  that  consonances  cause 
an  agreeable  kind  of  gentle  and  uniform  excitement  to  the  ear  which  is  distin- 
guished by  its  greater  variety  from  that  produced  by  a  single  compound  tone, 
whereas  the  sensation  caused  by  intermittent  dissonances  is  distressing  and  ex- 
hausting. 

However,  the  rule  that  the  various  parts  should  make  consonances  with  each 
other,  is  not  without  exception.  The  esthetic  reason  for  tliis  rule  is  not  opposed 
to  an  occasional  and  temporary  dissonance  among  the  various  parts,  provided  the 
motion  of  the  parts  is  so  contrived  as  to  make  the  directions  of  the  different  voices 
perfectly  easy  to  follow  by  the  ear.  Hence,  in  addition  to  the  general  laws  of  scale 
and.  key,  to  which  the  direction  of  every  part  is  subject,  there  are  particular  rulc^ 
for  the  progression  of  voices  through  discords.  Again,  dissonances  cannot  be  en- 
tirely excluded  because  consonances  arc  physically  more  agreeable.  That  which 
is  physically  agreeable  is  an  important  adjunct  and  support  to  esthetic  beauty,  but 

Digitized  by  V^OOQIC 


CHAP.  XVII.  DISSONANT  INTERVALS.  331 

it  is  certainly  not  identical  with  it.  On  the  contrary,  in  all  arts  we  frequently 
employ  its  opposite,  that  which  is  physically  disagreeable,  partly  to  bring  the 
beauty  of  the  first  into  relief,  by  contrast,  and  partly  to  gain  a  more  powerful 
means  for  the  expression  of  passion.  Dissonances  are  used  for  similar  purposes  in 
music.  They  are  partly  means  of  contrast,  to  give  prominence  to  the  impression 
made  by  consonances,  and  partly  means  of  expression,  not  merely  for  peculiar  and 
isolated  emotional  disturbances,  but  generally  to  heighten  the  impression  of  musical 
progress  and  impetuosity,  because  when  the  ear  has  been  distressed  by  dissonances 
it  longs  to  return  to  the  calm  current  of  pure  consonances.  It  is  for  this  last 
reason  that  dissonances  are  prominently  employed  immediately  before  the  conclu- 
sion of  a  piece,  where  they  were  regularly  introduced  even  by  the  old  masters  of 
medieval  polyphony.  But  to  effect  this  object  in  using  them,  the  motion  of  the 
parts  must  be  so  conducted  that  the  hearer  can  feel  throughout  that  the  parts  are 
pressing  forward  through  the  dissonance  to  a  following  consonance,  and,  although  T 
tliis  may  be  delayed  or  frustrated,  the  anticipation  of  its  approach  is  the  only  motive 
which  justifies  the  existence  of  the  dissonances. 

Since  any  relation  of  pitch  which  cannot  be  expressed  in  small  numbers  is 
dissonant,  and  it  is  only  the  number  of  the  consonances  which  is  limited,  the 
number  of  possible  dissonances  would  be  infinite  were  it  not  that  the  individual 
parts  composing  a  discord  in  music  must  necessarily  obey  the  laws  of  melodic 
motion,  that  is,  must  lie  within  the  scale.  Consonances  have  an  independent  right 
to  exist.  Our  modem  scales  have  been  formed  upon  them.  But  dissonances  are 
allowable  only  as  transitions  between  consonances.  They  have  no  independent 
right  of  existence,  and  tlie  parts  composing  them  are  consequently  obhged  to 
move  within  the  degree  of  the  scales,  by  the  same  laws  that  were  established  in 
favour  of  the  consonances. 

On  proceeding  to  a  detailed  consideration  of  the  separate  dissonant  intervals,  it 
should  be  rememberod  that  in  theoretical  music  the  normal  position  of  discords  is  H 
taken  to  be  that  which  arranges  their  tones  as  a  series  of  Thirds.  This,  for  example, 
is  the  rule  for  the  chord  of  the  dominant  Seventh,  which  consists  of  the  root,  its 
Third,  Fifth,  and  Seventh.  The  Fifth  forms  a  Third  with  the  Thurd,  and  the 
Seventh  forms  a  Third  with  the  Fifth.  Hence  we  can  consider  a  Fifth  to  be  com- 
posed of  two,  and  a  Seventh  of  three  Thirds.  By  inverting  Tliirds  we  obtain 
Sixths,  by  inverting  Fifths  we  obtain  Fourths,  and  by  inverting  Sevenths  we 
obtain  Seconds.     In  this  way  all  the  intervals  in  the  scale  are  reproduced. 

Using  the  present  modification  of  Hauptmann's  notation,  it  is  easily  seen  how 
different  intervals  of  the  same  name  must  differ  from  each  other  in  magnitude. 
We  have  only  to  remember  that  c^  is  a  comma  higher  than  0,  and  C|  two  commas 
lower  than  c^  and  one  comma  lower  than  c,  and  that  the  comma  is  about  the  fifth 
part  of  a  Semitone. 

To  obtain  a  general  view  of  both  the  magnitude  and  roughness  of  the  dissonant 
intervals,  I  have  constructed  fig.  6t  (p.  333a),  in  which  the  curve  of  roughness  is  % 
copied  from  fig.  60  A  (p.  1936).  The  base  line  X  Y  signifies  the  interval  of  an  Octave, 
upon  which  the  individual  consonant  and  dissonant  intervals  are  set  off  from  X, 
according  to  their  magnitude  on  this  scale.*  On  the  lower  side  of  the  base  are 
marked  the  twelve  equal  SemitcHies  of  the  equally-tempered  scale  [each  distant 
from  the  other  by  100  cents],  and  on  the  upper  side  the  consonant  and  dissonant 
intervals  which  occur  in  justly -intoned  scales.  The  magnitude  of  the  interval  is 
always  to  be  measured  on  the  base  line  from  X  to  the  corresponding  vertical  line. 
The  vertical  lines  corresponding  to  the  consonances  have  been  produced  to  tlie 
upper  margins  of  the  diagram,  and  those  for  the  dissonances  have  been  made 
shorter.  The  length  of  the  verticals  intercepted  between  the  base  and  the  curve 
of  roughness  shews  the  comparative  degree  of  roughness  probably  possessed  by  tlie 
interval  when  played  in  a  violin  quality  of  tone. 

♦  [That  is,  assuming  X  Y  to  represent  the  X  of  any  line  shewing  the  interyal,  givca  the 
cents  in  an  Octave  or  1200,  the  distance  from      centb  in  that  interval.  -Translator.] 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


332 


DISSONANT  INTERVALS. 


PART  III. 


*  [Tabclab  Expbbssiom  or  thb  Duobam,  Fio.  6i  Opposite. 


Intenrala 

No. 

Helmholtz's 

Notation,  as  in 

Diagram 

£lll«*s  Nototion 

of  InternUs 
reckoned  from  c 

Batio 

I  :  I 
I  :'^^2 

Cents 

Rouglincss 

Unison 

I 

c  :  C 

c :  C 

0 

0 

11    76 

Minor  Seconds 

2 

lie  :  ct 

lie :  ct 

II      100 

3 

br.c 

c  :db 

15  :  16 

112 

70 
38 

4 

die, 

c:d, 

9 :  10 

182 

Major  Seconds 

5 
6 

Herd 
cld 

\\c:d 
c:d 

i:'^ 
8:9 

11      200 
204 

0  25 
32 

7 
8 

6i  :  d'b 

c  :  e'^bb 

225  :  256 

224 

30 

a»b  :  6, 

c:d,u 

64  :  75 

274 

24 

Minor  Thirds 

9 

lO 

d:f 

Ijcrdg 

c  :  eb 

lie  :  dt 

27:32 
I  :  '^8 

294 

II      300 

26 

II  24 

II 

12 

♦c:c»b 

•cre'b 

♦5:6 

•    3'6 

♦  20 

•  8 

*c:e, 

♦c  :  e. 

♦4:5 

♦    386 

Major  Thirds 

13 

\\c  :  e  ' 

ic:e 

I :  »ri6 

II    400 

II  18 

14 

b,  :  e»'b 

clf'b 

25:32 

428 
*    498 

25 
*    2 

'5 

*c:/ 

*c:/ 

♦3:4 

Fourths 

i6 

lie:/ 

lie:/ 

1 :  'l'32 

«     500 

i     3 

17 
i8 

/».:d' 

ciP 

20:  27 

520 
568' 

27 

6'b:«'» 

clfji 

18:25 

32 

Sharp  Fourths 
or  Flat  Fifths 

19 

20 

|lc:/l 

c:/.» 

l|c:^ 

32:45 
i:^V64 

590 
II     600 

20 
H  18 

21 

6.:/ 

e :  gf'b 

45:64 

610 

28 

22 

«, :  6'b 

e:g'b 

25:36 

632 

35 

23 

d :  a, 

cifl'i 

27:40 

680 

44 

.   Fifths 

24 

lo:g 

Wcig 

I  :»Vi28 

II     700 

11     I 

25 

*c:g 

*c:g 

♦2:3 

•    702 
772 

♦    0 
39 

26 

e'b  :  6, 

c:g^ 

16 :  25 

Minor  Sixths 

27 

Ic :  (75 

He  :  gt 

1  :  ^^256 

1    800 

H  22 

28 

♦c :  a'b 

*c  :  a»b 

♦5:8 

*    814 

♦  20 

29 

*c:a. 

*c  :  a, 

•3:5 

♦    884 

♦    3 

Major  Sixths 

30 
31 

Ijc  :  a 
fid' 

lie:  a 
e  :  a 

I  :  i«/5i2 
16 :  27 

II     900 
906 

II  22 
24 

32 

6,  :  a'b 

c  :  6'bb 

75 :  128 

926 

24 

_ 

33 

d'b  :  6, 

c  :  a.,t 

128 :  225 
9 :  16 

976 
996 

15 

34 

d:c' 

c  :  db 

23 

Minor  Sevenths 

35 

l|c :  6b 

He  :  db 

I  :  »^I024 

II  1000 

II  24 

30 

e,:d' 

c  :  6'b 

5:9 

1018 

25 

Major  Sevenths 

37 
38 

c :  6, 
lie:  6 

e:6 
lie:  6 

8:15 

I : ^^^2048 

1088 
1  iioo 

42 
U  48 

•   Octave 

39 

♦c :  c' 

♦c:c' 

•i  :2 

*  1200 

»    0 

Digitized  by  VjOOQlC 


CIL\P.  XVII. 


DISSONANT   INTERVALS. 


333 


Pig.  6i.    (See  iwte  *  opposite,) 


The  preceding  tabular  expression  of  the 
diagram  will  be  often  found  eonvenient.  The 
degree  of  roughness  was  determined  by  mea- 
suring the  lengths  of  the  verticals  in  the  dia- 
gram in  hundredths  of  an  inch.  The  names  of 
the  notes  are  given  in  the  notation  of  the  text, 
using  superior  and  inferior  figures  for  the  lines 
above  and  below  in  the  diagram.  The  sign  || 
means  *  equally  tempered/  and  '*'  '  consonance.' 
The  cross  lines  group  the  just  intervals  repre- 
sented by  a  single  tempered  interval.  The 
cents  are  cyclical,  as  in  the  Duodenarium, 
App.  XX.  sect.  E.  art.  i8. 

The  intervals  in  the  diagram  are  not  noted 
as  from  C  to  another  tone,  but  as  between  the 
two  tones  where  they  usually  occur,  except  in 
the  equal  intonation  below.  In  the  Table  both 
are  given.  The  verticals  for  the  dissonances 
Were  placed  in  two  rows  in  re-cutting  the 
diagram  for  the  ist  edition  of  this  translation, 
pierely  for  the  purpose  of  clearness,  to  prevent 
the  letters  from  coming  too  close  to  each  other, 
but  without  attaching  any  meaning  to  the  differ- 
ence of  row ;  the  other  differences  described  in 
the  text  have  been  retained.  The  diagram  also 
uses  the  lines  above  and  below  the  letters  em- 
ployed in  the  ist  edition,  (p.  2770,  note  *)  and 
separates  the  letters  by  ( — ),  (p.  276^,  note  f) 
as  it  was  not  considered  advisable  to  re-engrave 
it.  In  the  Table,  however,  the  notation  of  the 
text  is  restored. 

Tabic  of  Roughness. 
The  following  is  a  comparative  arrangement 
of  these  intervals  in  order  of  roughness,  the 
consonances  being  marked  *,  and  the  tempered 
intervals  ||.  The  number  in  a  parenthesis  is 
that  of  the  interval  when  it  is  contained  in  the 
preceding  Table.  The  name  given  to  each  in- 
terval in  App.  XX.  sect.  D.  is  annexed,  fol- 
lowing by  its  roughness t  marked  *  ro.' 

Rough  iicffs 

*       o— (25)  just  Fifth. 


Roughness 

II       I — (24)  tempered  Fifth  representing  (25) 

just  Fifth,  ro.  o,  and  (23)  grave 

Fifth,  ro.  44. 

♦  2— (15)  just  Fourth. 

g  3— ( 16)  tempered  Fourth,  representing  ( 1 5)  _ 
just  Fourth,  ro.  2,  and  (17)  H 
acute  Fourth,  ro.  27. 

♦  3  -(29)  just  major  Sixth. 

♦  8— (12)  just  major  Third. 
15— (33)  extreme  sharp  Sixth. 

II  16 — (13)  tempered  major  Third,  represent- 
ing (12)  just  major  Third,  ro.  8, 
and  (14)  diminished  Fourth,  ro. 
25,— and  also  the  Pythagorean 
major  Third,  if  required,  ro.  19. 

II  18— (20)  tempered  sharp  Fourth  or  flat 
Fifth,  representing  (19)  false 
Fourth  or  Tritone,  ro.  20,  (21) 
diminished  Fifth,  ro.  28,  (18) 
superfluous  Fourth,  ro.  32,  and 
(22)  acute  diminished  Fifth, 
ro.  35. 
19— Pythagorean  major  Third  c  I  e  "SJ- 

408  cents.     See  p.  334,  note  J.  g| 

♦  20 — (28)  just  minor  Sixth.  " 

♦  20— (I I)  just  minor  Third. 

2a— (19)  false  Fourth  or  Tritone. 

II  22— (30)  tempered  major  Sixth,  represent- 
ing (29)  just  major  Sixth,  ro.  3, 
(31)  PyUiagorean  major  Sixth, 
ro.  24,  and  (32)  diminished 
Seventh,  ro.  24. 

II     22 — (27)  tempered  minor  Sixth,  represent- 
ing (28)   just  minor   Sixth,  ro. 
20,  and  (26)  grave  superfluous 
Fifth,  ro.  3Q. 
23 — (34)  minor  Seventh. 

II  24-  (35)  tempered  minor  Seventh,  repre- 
senting (33)  the  extreme  sharp 
Sixth,  ro.  15;  (34)  the  minor 
Seventh,  ro.  23,  and  (36)  the 
acute  minor  Seventh,  ro.  25. 
[Continued  on  next  page] 


Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


334 


THIRDS   AND   SIXTHS. 


PART  III. 


The  various  Thirds,  Fifths,  and  Sevenths  of  the  scale  are  foand  by  arranging 
it  in  Thirds  thus: — 

A.  Tones  op  the  MiUOR  Scale. 

hx-dlf+ai  —  c  +  Bi-g  +  bi'-dlf-ax 

5T     T         T        ¥         f 


32       A 
2T     ¥ 


B.  Tones  of  the  Minor  Scale. 
^r        l    i;     T^         ¥      ¥       IS 


A 


%\ 


For  the  minor  scale  I  have  assumed  the  usual  form  with  the  major  Seventh, 
^  because  scales  with  the  minor  Seventh  yield  the  same  intervals  as  the  major 
scale.* 

I.  Thirds  and  Sixths. 

The  above  schemes  shew  that  in  the  justly-intoned  major  and  minor  scales, 
three  kinds  of  Thirds  occur,  and  their  inversions  give  three  kinds  of  Sixths.   These 


are: 


i)  The  jiistly -intoned  major  Third  ^, 


inversion  the  minor  Sixth  |^, 


0 


[i2,  cents  386,  roughness  8],t  and  its 
[28,  cents  814,  roughness  20],  both  consonant. 
-Therjttstlf  intonod  mi;iior~ ThrcL  {jD  [11,  cents  316,  roughness  20],  and  its 
inversion  the  viajor  Sixth  f ,  [29,  cents  ^4,  roughness  3],  also  both  consonant. 

3)  The  Pythagorean  minor  Third  ^^,  [9,  cents  294,  roughness  26],  between 
the  extreme  tones  of  the  key,  d  and  /.  If  we  used  dy  in  place  of  d,  this  interval 
would  occur  between  by  and  d^.  On  comparing  this  dissonant  minor  Third  d  \f 
with  the  consonant  minor  Third  dy^f,  we  find  that  the  former  is  a  comma 
closer  than  the  latter,  since  d  ib  a,  comma  sharper  than  dy.  The  Pythagorean 
minor  Third  is  somewhat  less  harmonious  than  the  just  minor  Third,  but  tlie 
difference  between  them  is  not  so  great  as  that  between  the  two  corresponding 
major  Thirds.^  The  difference  of  the  two  cases  consists,  first,  in  the  major  Tliird 
being  a  much  more  perfect  consonance  than  the  minor  Third,  and  consequently 
much  more  liable  to  injury  from  defects  of  intonation ;  and  secondly  in  the  nature 


RouRhneas 

1}     24— (10)  tempered  minor  Third,  represent- 
ing (11)  just  minor  Third,  ro. 
20 ;  (8)  acute  augmented  Tone, 
ro.    24,    and    (9)    Pythagorean 
minor  Third,  ro.  26. 
24— (31)  Pythagorean  major  Sixth. 
24 —  Pythagorean  minor  Sixth  c  :  ab  -^ii 
=  792  cents. 
I        24 — (32)  diminished  Seventh. 
24—  (8)  augmented  Tone. 
II     25— (5)  tempered  major  Second  or  whole 
Tone,  representing  (7)  diminished 
minor  Third,  ro.  30,  (6)  major 
Tone,  ro.  32,  and  (4)  minor  Tone, 
ro.  38. 
25— (14)  diminished  Fourth. 
25 — (36)  acute  minor  Seventh. 
26 — (9)  Pythagorean  minor  Third. 
27— (17)  acute  Fourth. 
28— (21)  diminished  Fifth. 
29 — grave  major  Seventh  c  :  b^  =  {;?  -  1067. 
30 — (7)  diminished  minor  Third. ' 
32  —(6)  major  Tone. 


I  acute  diminished  Fifth. 


32  —  (18)  superfluous  Fourth^ 

3S-(22) 

38— (4)  minor  Tone. 

39— (26)  grave  superfluous  Fifth. 

42— (37)  just  major  Seventh. 


Roughness 

44— (2^)  grave  Fifth. 
ii    48  -{38)  tempered    major  Seventh,  repre. 
senting  (37)  just  major  Seventh, 
ro.  42. 

56— great  Limma  c  :  d*b  =f?  =  134  cents. 

70— (3)  just  minor  Second,  just  or  diatonic 
Semitone. 
II     76 — (2)   tempered    Semitone,   representing 
(3)    just    Semitone,    ro.    70. — 
Translator,] 

*  [The  remainder  of  this  chapter  should 
be  followed  step  by  step  on  the  Harmon ical, 
wherever  it  is  possible,  as  is  most  frequently 
the  case. — Translator.] 

f  [For  immediate  comparison  I  have,  after 
each  interval  as  it  arises,  inserted  in  square 
brackets,  the  number  of  the  interval,  the 
number  of  cents  it  contains,  and  its  degree  of 
roughness  as  given  in  the  Table  on  p.  332. 
— Translator.] 

X  [The  roughness  of  the  just  major  Third, 
c  +  e,  is  only  8,  while  that  of  the  Pythagorean  « 
l{  (which  is  not  given  in  the  Table  on  p.  332, 
becausie  it  does  not  occur  in  the  scale)  is 
necessarily  close  to  that  of  the  tempered  major 
Third,  18,  and  may  probably  be  taken  as  19, 
as  will  be  seen  by  the  curve  in  fig.  61,  p.  ;i^^. 
—  Translator.] 


Digitized  by  VjOOQlC 


CHAP.  XVII.  FIFTHS  AND  FOURTHS.  335 

of  the  two  combinational  tones.  The  just  minor  Third  d/"  — /"  has  6[>  for  its 
combinational  tone,  which  completes  it  into  the  just  major  triad  of  b\}.  The 
Pythagorean  minor  Third  d"'  \  f"  has  a,  for  its  combinational  tone,  which  com- 
pletes  it  into  the  chord  d\  f  •\-  a^^  and  this  is  not  a  perfectly  correct  minor  chord. 
But  as  the  incorrect  Fifth  a^  lies  among  the  deep  combinational  tones  and  is  very 
weak,  the  difference  is  scarcely  perceptible.  Moreover,  it  is  practically  almost  im- 
possible to  tune  the  interval  so  precisely  as  to  insure  the  combinational  tone  a^  in 
place  of  a.  But  for  the  Pythagorean  major  Third  c" .,  e"  the  combinational  tone 
is  (4f,  which  is,  of  course,  much  more  annoying  than  the  rather  imperfect  Fifth  a^ 
when  added  to  the  chord  d  \  f.* 

The  Pythagorean  major  Third  does  not  occur  in  scales  tuned  according  to  the 
conditions  of  harmonic  music.  If  we  used  the  minor  Seventh  b\}  in  place  of  ^^l> 
for  the  minor  scale,  b\}...d  would  be  a  Pythagorean  major  Third.t 

The  inversion  of  the  Third  ^  |/is  the  Pythagorean  major  Sixth  f...d\  f  J,  [31,  c 
cents  906,  roughness  24] »  which  is  a  comma  wider  than  the  just  major  Sixth,  and 
is  greatly  inferior  to  it  in  harmoniousness,  as  is  clearly  seen  in  fig.  61  (p.  333a). 

II.   Fifths  and  Fourths. 

The  Fifth  is  simply  composed  of  two  Thirds,  and  the  different  varieties  of 
Fifths  depend  upon  the  nature  of  those  Thirds. 

4)  The  just  Fifth  |,  [25,  cents  702,  roughness  o],  consists  of  a  just  major  and 
a  just  minor  Third,  or  f  =  J  x  ^  [cents  702  =  386  +  316].  Its  inversion  is  the 
just  J^ourth  f ,  [15,  cents  498,  roughness  2].  Both  are  consonant.  Examples  in 
the  major  scale, /±c',  ai  ±  e/,  c±^,  ei  ±&i,  ^±  d. 

5)  The  grave  or  imperfect  Fifth  d...ai  f^,  [23,  cents  680,  roughness  44],  a 
comma  [of  22  cents]  less  than  the  just  Fifth,  consists  of  a  Pythagorean  minor  and 

a  just  major  Third,  f  ?  =  ^f  x  f  [cents  680  =  294  +  386].  It  sounds  like  a  badly-  ^ 
tuned  Fifth,  and  makes  clearly  sensible  beats.  In  the  Octave  C'...&\  the  number 
of  these  beats  in  a  second  is  11.  Its  inversion,  the  acute  or  imperfect  Fourth, 
ai...d\  f  J,  [17,  cents  520,  roughness  27],  is  also  decidedly  dissonant.  The  Fourth 
Ai...d  makes  as  many  beats  in  a  second  as  the  Fifth  d...aiy  the  d  being  the  same 
in  each,  [see  App.  XX.  sect.  G.  art.  16]. 

6)  The  false  or  diminished  Fifth,  bi..,  /,  ^,  [21,  cents  610,  roughness  28], 
consists  of  a  just  and  Pythagorean  minor  Third,  f|  =  J  x  ^,  [cents  610  =  316  + 
294]  and  is,  hence,  as  the  composition  shews,  [92  cents  or]  about  half  a  Tone  closer 
than  the  just  Fifth.  It  is  a  tolerably  rough  dissonance,  nearly  equal  in  roughness 
to  a  major  Second  [6,  cents  204,  roughness  32].  Its  inversion  is  the  false  Fourth 
or  Tritone,  /...  61,  ^,  [19,  cents  590,  roughness  20],  consisting  of  three  whole 
Tones,  major/...gf,minorgf...a,,and  major  ai...6,,  f  x  V  x  |  =  ^,  [cents  590  = 
204  +  182  -f  204] ;  it  has  very  nearly  the  same  degree  of  roughness  as  the  last  [or 
false  Fifth],  and  is  [20  cents  or]  about  a  comma  closer.  For  the  false  Fifth  ^1./^ 
is  nearly  the  same  as  c|>.. ./,  and  if  we  diminish  this  interval  by  a  comma  we  obtain 
cl>  — /i,  which  is  a  false  Fourth.  Strictly  speaking,  as  c\}  is  not  precisely  the  same 
as  5 1,  the  difference  between  the  intervals  is  not  precisely  a  comma,  |^,  but  about 
^,  [or  1^  of  a  comma  =  20  cents].     On  keyed  instruments  they  coincide. 

7)  The  superfliums  or  extreme  sharp  Fifth  of  the  minor  scale,  e*t>  -^u  t¥>  [26, 
cents  772,  roughness  39],  consists  of  two  major  Thirds,  e^\}  +  g,  and  ^  +  6,,  ^  = 
J  X  f  [cents  772  =  386  -f  386].  It  is  seen  to  be  [42  cents  or]  nearly  two  commas 
[44  cents]  closer  than  the  minor  Sixth,  [cents  814]  by  putting  for  b-  the  nearly 
identical  ol>,  so  that  e^t>...5,  is  nearly  the  same  as  e^|>...c[>,  whereas  the  consonant 
minor  Sixth  is  e||;>...c|;>,  where  «it>  is  two  commas  flatter  than  e^\}.  The  superfluous 
Fifth  [26,  cents  772,  roughness  39]  is  markedly  rougher  than  the  minor  Sixth  [28, 

*  [In  just  intonation,  however,  the  difference  f  [The  Pythagorean  major  Third  of  408 

between  <2,  — /  and  d  \  f  i&  very  marked,  as  cents  does  not  occur  on  the  Harmonical.  The 
may  be  readily  observed  on  the  Harmonical. —  nearest  interval  '&b...(i,  of  413  cents  is  supe- 
Translator.]  rior  in  eflect.^Tra7tslator.] 

Digitized  by  VjOOQlC 


336  SEVENTHS   AND   SECONDS.  p.uiT  iii. 

cents  814,  roughness  20],  with  wliich  it  coincides  upon  keyed  instruments.  Its  in- 
version, the  diminished  Fourth  6,...e*'t>»  ^I»  [14,  cents  428,  roughness  25],  is  [42 
cents  or]  about  two  commas  higher  than  the  just  major  Third,  [12,  cents  386, 
rouglmess  8],  and  considerably  rougher,  although  the  two  intervals  coincide  on 
keyed  instruments,  [13,  cents  400,  common  rouglmess  18]. 

Two  just  or  two  Pythagorean  minor  Thirds  cannot  occur  consecutively  in  the 
natural  series  of  Thirds  of  the  just  major  and  minor  scales.  In  the  modes  of  the 
minor  Seventh  and  of  the  Fourth,  we  may  find  the  intervals  a|...e*|;>  and  ex...b^\} 
=  JJ,  [22,  cents  632,  roughness  35],  composed  of  two  minor  Tliirds,  ^f  =  J  x  f 
[cents  632  •—  316  H-  316] ;  these  are  a  comma  wider  than  the  usual  false  Fifths 
6,../  (or  ap.  e'\}  m  the  key  of  b\}  major,  and  <j,...6t>  in  the  key  of /major),  and 
are  decidedly  rougher  than  these,  [21,  cents  610,  roughness  28]. 


III.   Sevenths  and  Seconds. 

Any  three  successive  Thirds  give  a  Seventh.  Beginning  with  the  smallest  we 
obtam  the  following  different  magnitudes  : 

8)  The  diminished  Seventh  of  the  minor  scale  fep..a*'t>  [32,  cents  926,  rough- 
ness 24],  =  (6,  —  d')  +  (rf'  +/)  H-  (/  —  a*'l>),  or  two  just  minor  Thirds  and  one 
Pythagorean  minor  Third.  Its  numerical  ratio  is  VV  =  ir  ^  5t  ^  J,  [cents  926  = 
316  +  294  -f-  316,]  which  is  [42  cents  or]  about  two  commas  greater  than  tlie  major 
Sixth  [29,  cents  884,  roughness  3],  as  is  seen  by  putting  6,  ...a*'t>  =  c[>..  a*'l>.  The 
interval  c\^...a\'\}^  which  is  two  commas  flatter  than  the  last,  would  be  a  just 
major  Sixth.  Its  dissonance  is  harsh  and  rough,  the  same  as  that  of  the  Pytha- 
gorean major  Sixth  c.  a,  [31,  cents  906],  which  is  [20  cents  or]  about  a  conmia 
less.    But  its  inversion,  the  superfiuoiLs  Second  a^\}...hx  [8,  cents  274,  having  tlie 

K  same  roughness  24],  is  not  much  rougher  than  the  just  minor  Third  [i  i,  cents  316, 
roughness  20 ;  the  tempered  minor  Third  10,  cents  300,  has  exactly  the  same 
roughness  24].  Its  numerical  ratio  7ff  [c®ii*s  274]  is  very  nearly  J  [cents  267] 
(since^  =  |  x  5.JJ  [cents  274=  267  -f  7]).  If  this  Second  is  extended  to  a  Ninth, 
7,^,  [Ijaving  1474  cents  or]  nearly  ^,  [cents  1467]  it  becomes  tolerably  harmonious, 
as  much  so  as  the  minor  Tenth  V»  [cents  15 16]  which,  however,  is  a  very  imper- 
fect consonance,  [see  fig.  60  B,  p.  193c].* 

9)  The  closer  minor  Seventh  of  the  scale  g-'-fj  bx  .  a/,  or  d—c\  V,  [34,  cents 
996,  roughness  23],  consists  of  a  just  major,  a  just  minor,  and  a  Pythagorean 
minor  Third,  ^  -  /  =  (  ^  +  &,)  +  (6,  -  ci')  +  (d''  -  /),  [or  V  =  ^  x  f  x  5| 
cents  996  =  386  -I-  316  +  294.]  It  is  a  comparatively  mild  dissonance,  milder  than 
the  diminished  Seventh  [32,  cents  926,  roughness  24],  and  this  is  of  importance 
for  the  effect  of  the  chord  of  the  dominant  Seventh,  in  which  the  Seventh  has 
this  form.     This  closer  minor  Seventh  is  the  interval  of  a  Seventh  in  the  scale 

H  nearest  to  the  natural  Seventh  or  seventh  harmonic,  J  [=  V  x  JJ,  cents.  969  =-996 
—  27],  although  not  so  close  as  the  extreme  sharp  Sixth  [33,  cents  976=996-  20, 
roughness  15].  It  has  been  already  shewn  that  the  natural  Seventh  belongs  to 
harmonious  combinations  (pp.  195a,  217c).  The  inversion  of  this  Seventh  is  the 
major  Second,  c...d,  ai,..by,  /.,  g,  |,  [6,  cents  204,  roughness  32],  a  powerful 
dissonance. 

10)  The  actcte  or  wider  minor  Seventh,  e,...rf',  ai...g',  ^,  [36,  cents  1018, 
roughness  25] ,  a  comma  greater  than  the  last,  is  distinctly  harsher  than  that  interval, 
because  it  is  nearer  to  the  Octave  ;  its  roughness  [25]  is  nearly  the  same  as  that  of 
tlie  diminished  Seventh  [24].  It  consists  of  a  just  major  and  two  just  minor 
Thirds :  e^.^.d'  =  (e,  -  g)  +  (g  >  b^)  -f-  (b^  -  d'),  [or  J  =  ^  x  ^  x  f ,  cents  1018 
=  316  +  386  +  316.]     The  last-mentioned  closer  minor  Seventh  has  its  root  on  the 

*  [Compare  on  Harmonical  a''b...6,"  with      g\..b^"b.    The  (/'... '6"b  will  be  found  much 
l/'...'6"b,  and  a'b...6/  with  g^..,^b"\>  and  with       the  most  hannonious.—  rrawsfa/cw.;; 


Digitized  by  V^OOQIC 


CHAP.  XVII.    MAJOR  SEVENTH  AND  EXTBEME  SHARP  SIXTH,         337 

dominant  side  of  the  scale,  and  its  Seventh  on  the  subdominant  side,  because  it 
contains  the  Pythagorean  minor  Third  d  \  /.  The  wider  minor  Seventh,  on  the 
other  hand,  has  its  Seventh  on  the  dominant  side.  Its  inversion,  the  minor  Tone, 
^f  d...ei,  g...aif  [4,  cents  182,  roughness  38]  is  somewhat  harsher  than  the  major 
Tone  [6,  cents  204,  roughness  32]. 

11)  The  major  Seventh  f...e\,  c.hy,  */,  [37,  cents  1088,  roughness  42],  con- 
sists of  two  just  major  and  one  just  minor  Third  :  C...5,  =  (c  +  ei)  +  (^i  —  g)  + 
{g  +  bi)  [or  V  =  T  X  I  ><  T»  cents  1088  =  386  -f  316  +  386].  It  is  a  harsh  dis- 
sonance, about  the  same  as  the  minor  Tone  [4,  cents  182,  roughness  38].  Its 
inversion,  the  minor  Second  or  Semitone  ^i-.c',  ei.../,  |f,  [3,  cents  112,  roughness 
70],  is  the  harshest  dissonance  in  the  scale.* 

In  the  mode  of  the  Fourth  or  minor  Seventh,  we  find  a  somewhat  closer 
major  Seventh,  b^\}.,.a\,  which  is  a  comma  closer  than  the  usual  major  Seventh, 
and  hence  somewhat  milder  in  effect.t  H 

Finally  we  have  to  mention  an  interval  peculiar  to  the  Doric  mode  of  the 
minor  Sixth,  namely : 

12)  The  sujperfltious  or  extreme  sharp  Sixth  d^\}...bi,  which  arises  in  this  mode 
from  connecting  the  peculiar  minor  Second  of  the  mode  d^\}  with  the  leading  note 
bi  [see  p.  2S6b]. 

The  numerical  ratio  is  f||,  [33,  cents  976,  roughness  15],  so  that  it  is  [20  cents  or] 
about  a  comma  less  than  the  closer  minor  Seventh  of  the  chord  of  the  dominant 
Seventh  [cents  996],  as  is  seen  by  putting  d2*t>...  6,  =c^^t>--c'l>;  the  interval  d\^  .c'\} 
would  be  the  closer  minor  Seventh,  and  d^\}  is  a  comma  higher  than  d\}.  The 
superfluous  Sixth  may  be  conceived  as  composed  of  two  just  major  Thirds  and 
one  just  major  Tone :  {d^\} +f)  +  (f...g)  +  (S' +  ^'i)  =  ^*b  ...  &i,  [or|?i  =  f  x  f 
X  f ,  cents  976  =  386  +  204  +  386].  Its  harmoniousness  is  equal  to  that  of  the 
minor  Sixth,  because  it  is  almost  exactly  the  natural  Seventh  ^^i  since  i||= 7  x 
Hf  [or  976  =  969  +  7]-  Taken  alone  it  cannot  be  regarded  as  a  dissonance,  but  H 
it  makes  no  other  consonant  combinations,  and  hence  is  unfit  for  use  in  consonant 
chords.  When  it  is  inverted  into  the  diminished  Third  |ff  [cents  224],  or  nearly 
^  [cents  231],  it  is,  as  already  observed,  considerably  damaged  [7,  cents  224,  the 
roughness  rises  to  30],  but  it  is  improved  by  taking  the  upper  tone  bi  an  Octave 
higher,  in  which  case  it  is  [cents  2176  or]  nearly  ^  [=  cents  2169].  Its  near 
agreement  with  the  natural  Seventh  and  its  comparative  harmoniousness,  seem  to 
have  preserved  this  remarkable  interval  in  certain  cadences,  although  it  is  quite 
foreign  to  our  present  tonal  system.  It  is  characteristic  that  musicians  forbid  its 
inversion  into  the  diminished  Third  (which  lessens  its  harmoniousness),  but  allow 
its  extension  into  the  corresponding  Thirteenth  (which  improves  its  harmonious- 
ness). On  keyed  instruments  this  interval  coincides  with  the  minor  Seventh  [35, 
cents  1000,  roughness  24]. 

Generally,  a  glance  at  fig.  61  (p.  333a)  will  shew  to  what  an  extraordinary  extent 
different  intervals  are  fused  on  keyed  instruments.§     On  the  lower  side  of  the  base  ^ 
line  X  Y  are  marked  the  places  of  the  tones  of  the  equally  tempered  scale,  and  the 
small  braces  below  the  base  line  shew  those  different  tonal  degrees  which  are 

*  [That  is  in  the  jast  major  scale;  the  1  =  969  cents.     As  a  matter  of  fact,  on  my 

8emitone  of  the  tempered  scale,  2,  reaches  76  meantone  concertina  I  find  /  966  d%  much 

degrees  of  roughness. — Translator.]  smoother  than  /  1007  eb.    The  chord  intro- 

t   [Its  numerical  ratio  is  ^  =  V'  ^  a?*  cents  ducing  this  interval  occurs  in  three  forms. 

1088 -22  =1066,  so  that  it  is  the  interval  c...6g,  The  'Italian'  D'b  386  F  590   J5„  and  the 

which  by  fig.  61  (p.  333a)  should  have  a  rough-  *  German  *  D»b  386  F  316  A^b  274  J5,,  are 

ness  of  about  29,  in  place  of  42,  the  roughness  simply  imitations  of  the  true  chord  of  the 

of  c..,b^.— -Translator.]  dominant  Seventh  D»b   386  F  316  A»b  267 

J  [The  diagram,  fig.  61  (p.  333a),  gives  the  'C*b.  The  *  French '  form,  (the  only  one  con- 
roughness  of  the  superfluous  Sixth  as  15,  and  sidered  in  the  text  and  on  p.  2866,)  Z>'b  386 
that  of  the  minor  Sixth  as  20 ;  see  p.  3330^,  d'.  F  204  O  386  B^  is  the  harshest  of  all.  The  O 
This  would  make  the  former  more  harmonious  seems  to  be  merely  an  anticipation  of  the  note 
tiian  the  latter.  This  interval  does  not  exist  on  of  the  chord  C  316  -B'b  386  G  498  c  on  which 
the  Harmonical.  In  meantone  intonation,  the  it  resolveB.— Translator.] 
extreme  sharp  Sixth  has  only  966  cents,  and  is  §  [This  is  shewn  in  detail  on  pp.  332-4,  note, 
therefore  still  closer  to  the  subminor  Seventh  -^Translator,] 

Digitized  by  VjO@QlC 


338  DISSONANT  TKIADS-  pabt  in. 

usually  expressed  by  the  corresponding  tone  of  the  tempered  scale.  The  interval 
bi...a^\}  [cents  926]  is  identified  on  the  pianoforte  with  the  major  Sixth  c\}...a\} 
[cents  884,  or  42  cents  closer],  while  the  interval  d^\}...bi  [cents  976,  or  50  cents 
wider  than  the  first]  is  made  a  (tempered)  Semitone  [cents  100]  wider  [being 
identified  with  1000  cents],  and  yet  the  last  is  scarcely  more  different  from  the 
first,  than  the  first  from  a  major  Sixth.  The  figure  61  shews  also  very  clearly 
what  an  immense  difference  of  harmoniousness  ought  to  exist  between  the  first 
and  either  of  the  two  last  of  the  following  intervals  c.ai,  /...d',  and  bi.,.a^\}, 
[29,  31,  32,  respective  cents  884,  906,  926,  respective  roughness  3,  24,  24], 
which  are  all  expressed  by  the  sufficiently  harsh  sound  of  the  tempered  inter- 
val c.,.a  [30,  cents  900,  roughness  22].  The  justly-intoned  harmonium  with  two 
rows  of  keys*  allows  all  these  intervals  to  be  given  accurately,  by  which  the  differ- 
ence of  their  sound  becomes  extremely  striking.  In  this  evidently  lies  one  of  the 
IT  greatest  imperfections  of  tempered  intonation. 

Dissonant  Triads. 

Dissonant  triads  with  a  single  dissonant  interval  are  obtained  by  taking  two 
tones  which  are  consonant  to  the  root,  but  dissonant  to  each  other.    Thus  : 

i)  Fifth  and  Fourth  :  c...f...g,  [orfdtc±g], 

2)  Third  and  Fourth  :  c  +  ej.../ or  c  —  e^\}   ./,  [or/±  c  +  Bi  and/±  c— «*W. 

3)  Fifth  and  Sixth :  c  ±  gr  .  a,  or  c  ±:g.,.a^\},  [or  a—c  ±  g,  and  a^\}  -\-  c:t.g], 

4)  Dissimilar  Thirds  and  Sixths:  c— c'|:>...ai  or  c  +  ei...a*l>,  [orai— c— e"l> 
anda^b  +  c  +  «i].t 

In  all  these  c  is  consonant  with  each  of  the  other  two  tones.  The  first  chord 
alone  plays  a  great  part  in  the  older  polyphonic  music  as  a  chord  of  suspension, 
%  The  others  we  shall  meet  with  again  in  the  chords  of  the  Seventh. 

The  chords  named  in  the  fourth  series  above  t  admit  of  an  inversion  which 
makes  them  appear  as  triads  with  diminished  or  superfluous  Fifths,  namely  : 

ai  —  c  —  e^\}  and  a^\}  +  c  +  gj. 

The  first  of  these  is  composed  of  two  just  minor  Thirds,  [so  that  the  Fifth 
ax,..e^\},  No.  22,  ratio  25  :  36,  cents  632,  roughness  35,  is  the  acute  diminished 
Fifth,]  and  the  second  of  two  just  major  Thirds,  [so  that  the  Fifth  a^\}...ei.  No.  26, 
ratio  25  :  16,  cents  772,  roughness  39,  is  the  grave  superfluous  Fifth],  Both  are 
dissonant  on  account  of  the  altered  Fifth,  although  the  dissonance  of  the  second 
has  to  be  played  as  the  consonance  ^2$... e  [minor  Sixth  814  cents]  upon  keyed 
instruments.  The  first  of  these  two  chords  can  only  appear  in  the  mode  of  the 
minor  Third,  and  the  above  would  be  heard  in  that  of  F.§  The  second,  on  the 
other  hand,  belongs  £0  J^  minor.** 
5F       If  we  suppose  this  series  of  tones  to  be  continued  as 

a»l>  +  c'+  e/...  a»'t>  +  c"+  e/' 

i    i    n      i    i 

*  [And,  with  the  exception  of  the  extreme  —  c— 6*b  +  g,  that  is  in  one  of  the  forms 

^rp  Sixth  d'b...6,,on  the  Harmonic^  also.  6b  +  d,-/+a,-c-e»b  +  ^=i  Fma.maanL 

The  extreme  sharp  Sixth  c...a^  may  be. ^^  ftb-d'biz+a^-c-e^b  +  i^^i  FmijnaanL 

oiently  imitated  as  c.Jhb.^Translatar.']  /+a,-c-e»b  +  ^  +  6,-d  =  i  C  ma.mi.ma. 

t  [These  triads  I  propose  to  term  can^^  /+a  -c-<i«b  +  g-6'b  +  d-i  Cma.mi.mL 

sonant^  and  the  two  last  especially  I  caU  the  ''  ' 

minor  and  major  trine.    See  App.  XX.  sect.  E.  Bat  not  one  of  these  belongs  to  the  mode  of 

art.  5. — Translator.']  the  minor  Third,  which  for  F  is  i  F  mijnijni., 

X  [From  p.  338c,  beginning  with  these  words  unless  the  second  is  taken  to  be  such  with 

to  the  paragraph  ending  *  as  in  concords,'  on  a  major  tonic.     The  last,  however,  is  the 

p.  339&,  is  an  insertion  in  the  4th  German  edi-  mode  of  the  minor  Seventh  of  C — TraMS- 

tion.  —Translator,]  latorJ] 

§  [It  is  evident  that  a,— c— e'b  can  only  **  [In  the  major  dominant  fonn  6  b—cl^b-f 

occur  when  the  chain  of  chords  contains  / + a,  /— a'  b  +  e  +  61 — g. — Translaior.] 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


CHAP.  xvn.  DISSONANT  TRIADS.  339 

an  interval  glides  in  of  §|  =  f  •  f|f  =  J  •  ||  approximatively  [cents  428  ==  386 + 42] , 
which  is  slightly  (about  2  commas)  greater  than  a  just  major  Third.  By  small 
alterations  of  pitch  other  chords  are  formed  which  belong  to  other  keys  : 


A^\}  +  c  +  61  .,.a^\}  in  i'' minor 

Oiij^,.,c  +  ei  +  gjj^  in  -4,  minor 

U   i       i 
A^\}  +  c.../'t>+a^t>  ^  -D*t>  minor 

The  roots  of  these  three  minor  keys 

D'\}  +  F+A, 

form  a  similar  chord,  of  which  the  roots  are  a  Semitone  higher  than  those  of  the  ^ 
preceding.*  Since  ^*|;>  is  nearly  the  same  as  G^jf^,  and  F^\}  nearly  the  same  as 
£ji,  these  transformations  alter  the  pitch  of  one  of  the  tones  in  the  chord  by 
about  two  commas,  or,  at  least  in  the  resolution  of  the  chord,  this  tone  is  treated 
as  a  leading  note  just  as  if  it  were  thus  altered.  Hence  we  obtain  modulations 
which  with  a  single  step  lead  us  to  comparatively  distant  keys,  and  we  can  as 
easily  resolve  into  the  minor  as  into  the  major  keys  of  the  three  roots  named. 
This  means  of  modulation  is  often  employed  by  modem  composers,  (for  example 
B.  Wagner)  in  place  of  using  the  chord  of  the  diminished  Seventh,  which  is  much 
rougher  but  was  also  applied  for  the  same  purpose.  In  just  intonation  these 
chords  are  not  by  any  means  so  unpleasant  as  in  the  tempered  intonation  of  the 
pianoforte.  Generally  it  may  be  observed  that  when  one  is  accustomed  to  play  in 
just  intonation,  the  ear  becomes  quite  as  sensitive  to  a  pitch  which  is  wrong  by  a 
comma  in  discords  as  in  concords. 

For  modem  music  triads  with  two  dissonances,  formed  by  including  the  ^ 
extremes  of  the  key,  are  more  important. 

In  the  series  of  chords  in  any  key,  major  and  minor  Thirds  follow  each  other 
alternately,  and  any  two  adjacent  Thirds  produce  a  consonant  triad.  But  the 
interval  between  the  extreme  tones  d  and  /  is  a  Pythagorean  minor  Third,  and 
when  these  are  connected  as  a  chord  with  one  of  either  of  the  two  adjacent  tones 
to  make  a  new  triad,  it  will  be  dissonant. 

Major :  o  +  Bi    — gr  +  61— i|/+a,    —c  +  ei    ^g 


Minor  :  c  —  e*b  +  gr  +  61  —  d  \  f—  a^\}  -^  c  —  e^\}  +  g 

far,  83S6  06  A 

T      1       f     VT    f  Tir         ^ 

The  major  system  gives  two  triads  of  this  kind : 

bi-d+f  and  d  I  /+  ai  % 

in  Mi 

The  minor  scale  also  gives  two  : 

bi-d  \f  and  d  \  f  -  a^\> 

i  u  n  i 

In  the  two  triads  bi—  d  \  /and  d  \  f  —  a^|?,  which  combine  a  Pythagorean 
with  a  just  minor  Third,  there  are  also  second  dissonances,  namely  the  false  Fifths 
&!.../ and  d...a^\}f  which  make  the  chord  more  strongly  dissonant  than  the  Pytha- 
gorean minor  Third  ff  alone  could  make  them.    They  are  hence  called  diminished 

*  [Only  in  the  form  c  +  6, +^^.     From  apart,  and  could  not  pOBsibly  be  confounded  in 

what  follows  it  is  evident  that  the  transforma-  just  intonation.    Of  course  Wagner  thought 

tion  could  only  take  place  in  tempered  intona-  only  in  equal  temperament,  in  which  the  tones 

tion.    The  tones  confounded  are  all  42  cents  are  absolutely  identical.— Tmn^Zator.] 

Digitized  by  V^(^€>Q1C 


340 


DISSONANT  TRIADS. 


FABT  nL 


triads^  The  chord  d  |  /  -f  ^i,  which  in  the  usual  musical  notation  is  not  distiii* 
guished  from  the  minor  triad  c^i  —  /  +  aj,  and  may  hence  be  called  the  false  miner 
triad,  is,  as  Hauptmann  has  correctly  shewn,  dissonant,  and  on  justly-intoned 
instruments  it  is  very  decidedly  dissonant.  It  sounds  almost  as  rough  as  the  chord 
6,  —  (i  I  /.  If  in  G  major,  without  confounding  d  with  di,  we  form  the  cadence 
I  or  2 
r,    -  ^j        ^  -[0]       _  [sodded] 


lE^^ 


1"- 

the  chords  a^^...d"  \  f  and/  -f  ai',,.d"  \  f  are  quite  as  dissonant  in  their  effects 
as  the  following  hi'  --  d"  \f  and  g'  -»-  hi  —  d"  \  f".  But  on  account  of  the  in- 
correct intonation  of  our  musical  instruments  we  cannot  produce  the  same  effect 

^  without  combining  an  inverted  chord  of  the  Seventh  with  the  subdominant  in  the 
cadence,  as  /+  ai  —  d ,..d\  Hauptmann  doubts  whether  in  practice  the  iialse 
minor  chord  of  the  key  of  Q  major  can  be  distinguished  from  the  minor  chord  of  D. 
I  find  that  this  is  most  distinctly  and  undoubtedly  effected  on  my  justly  intoned 
harmonium,  but  allow  that  we  cannot  expect  the  correct  intonation  from  singers. 
They  will  involuntarily  pass  into  the  minor  chord,  unless  the  progression  of  the 
parts  which  execute  D,  strongly  emphasise  its  connection  with  the  dominant  G.* 
These  chords,  and  among  them  most  decisively  and  distinctly  the  chord 
hi  —  d  Iff  have  for  musical  composition  the  especially  important  advantage  of 
combining  those  limiting  tones  of  the  key,  which  separate  it  from  the  nearest 
related  keys,  and  are  consequently  extremely  well  suited  for  marking  the  key  in 
which  the  harmony  is  moving  at  any  given  time.  If  the  harmony  passed  into  G 
major  or  G  minor,  /  would  have  to  be  replaced  by /i  J.  If  it  passed  into  -F  major, 
d  would  become  di  and  if  into  F  minor  d  would  become  d^\}  and  Z>,  would  in  the 

^  same  chords  become  h^\}.    Thus — 


in  G  major  : 

b,  -d  +/,# 

d     +/,#-« 

in  C  major  : 

b,  -d   \f 

d      1  /     +  a, 

in  F  major  : 

b\>  +d,  -f 

di    -/     +a, 

in  G  minor  : 

b'\>  +  d     +/,# 

d     +  /,#  -  a 

in  C  minor  : 

b,    -d      1/ 

d      1/     -a't) 

in  F  minor  : 

b\}   -d'\}+/ 

d'\}+f     -a't) 

This  shews  that  the  chords  in  the  nearest  related  keys  are  all  distinctly  different, 
with  the  exception  of  d  \f  +  ai  and  ^i  —  /  +  ^i,  the  distinction  between  which  in 


♦  [The  chord  on  the  Second  of  the  major 
scale  is  in  fact  the  crux  of  the  translation  of 
tempered  into  just  intonation.    It  is  easy  to 
^  play  Ex.  I  and  2,  and  Ex.  3,  here  added,  as 

a'    d^'   f         f    a'    d,"  f        f  a(  d," 

6/  d"    /'andfl'    6/     d"   /'and^  V  d" 

d'    c"     e,"        c'    c"     c"     e,"      f  6/  d" 

e'  d'  d' 

and  the  effect  is  not  bad.  In  the  first  the  d(' 
might  be  held  on  to  the  second  chord,  as  6'  d" 
f\  without  materially  increasing  the  harshness 
of  the  dissonance,  but  in  the  second  this  would 
give  ^  6/  d/'  /",  where  the  grave  Fifth  is  very 
harsh.  In  the  second  case,  then,  there  is 
least  harshness  in  playing  d!'  in  both  chords. 
And  in  both  cases  there  is  most  smoothness 
in  playing  them  as  just  written.  The  effect  is 
one  on  which  I  have  repeatedly  experimented, 
'but  I  find  that  the  smali  interval  d^'  d"  in  the 
highest  or  lowest  part,  produces  a  strange  effect, 
which  in  singing,  and  perhaps  on  the  violin, 
seems  to  be  overcome  by  a  gUde,  if  the  other 


voices  are  strong  enough  to  pull  this  voice  out 
of  its  course,  even  though  the  words  and  parts 
are  written  so  as  to  imply  that  this  note  is  sus- 
tained. WTien  the  d"  is  in  the  principal  pari 
in  the  melody,  as  in  the  third  example,  I  find 
it  best  on  the  whole  not  to  play  as  written,  d/' 
d'\  but  to  sustain  d'\  In  some  cases  an  at- 
tempt to  avoid  the  dissonance,  which  is  really 
harsh,  would  lead  to  such  melodic  phrases  as 
d  d,  d,  which  would  be  simply  impossible  for 
an  unaccompanied  voice.  If  in  the  third  ex- 
ample d"  were  held  throughout,  and  the  ac- 
companying voices  sang  the  minor  chord,  wa 
should  get  the  succession  P'a'd",  g'  6/  d*\  f  b/ 
d'\  e*  c"  c\  which  amounts  to  a  modulatioa 
into  the  minor  of  the  dominant,  instead  of 
into  the  subdominant.  Whether  such  is  pos- 
sible depends  on  the  preceding  chords.  As/* 
does  not  occur  on  the  Harmonical,  I  played 
Ex.  3  on  my  just  concertina  in  A^  major  as  d' 
fx't  W,  e/  g^t  6/,  d,'  g^l  6/,  Cj'J  a/  a\  and 
found  that  such  chords  produced  the  best  effect 
of  all  for  this  isolated  phrase. — TraJisl(UoT.\ 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


CHAP.  XVII.  CHORDS  OF  THE   SEVENTH.  341 

singing  might  be  doubtful.   The  rest  are  much  more  clearly  distinguished  from  the 
chords  in  the  nearest  adjacent  keys.     Nevertheless 

b^-^d    \  f  and        d  \  f  -a'\} 

6  33  3  2  8 

5       TT  irr      f 

axe  easily  confused  with 

&,   I  d, -/         and        d-f  I  a»t> 


U       i  i       ^ 


I 


of  which  the  former  belongs  to  Ai  minor,  and  the  latter  to  E^\}  major  or  minor, 
where  A^  minor  is  the  minor  key  nearest  related  to  C  major,  and  E^\}  major  is  the 
major  key  nearest  related  to  C  minor. 

Finally  when  we  remember  that  the  Pythagorean  minor  Third  ff  [cents  294]  ^ 
is  nearer  the  superfluous  second  J  J  [cents  274]  than  to  the  normal  minor  Third 
[cents  316]  (^?  =  5  X  Sy   [cents  294  =  316  —  22]  and  Jf  =  J-J  x  fJ^J  [cents  294 
=  274  +  20]  or  nearly  =  H  ^  f  3^»  ^^  requires  comparatively  slight  changes  in 
intonation  to  convert  the  chord  bi—  d  \  /into 

by  —  d  ...CjiJ       and        c^}[).,.di—f 

6  7.'}  70  n 

which  belong  to  i^jjlj!  minor  and  E\)  minor.  Hence  the  diminished  triad  bi  —  d  |  /, 
by  sUght  changes  of  intonation,*  never  exceeding  |^,  can  be  referred  to  the  keys 
of 

C  major,  C  minor,  Ai  minor,  F^jj^  minor,  and  E\}  minor. 

Hence  although  the  use  of  the  diminished  triad  61  —  ^  |  /  excludes  the  keys  f 
most  nearly  related  to  C,  it  allows  of  a  confusion  with  more  distant  keys,  and  hence 
also  the  characterisation  of  the  key  by  these  triads  will  not  be  complete  without  a 
fourth  note,  converting  the  triad  into  a  tetrad.     This  leads  us  to  the  chords  of  the 
Seventh  proper. 

Chords  of  the  Seventh. 

A.  Formed  of  two  Consonant  Triads, 

Consonant  tetrads,  or  chords  in  four  parts,  as  shewn  on  p.  222^,  cannot  be 
constructed  without  using  the  Octave  of  one  of  the  tones,  but  dissonant  tetrads  are 
easily  constructed.  The  least  dissonant  of  such  chords  are  those  in  which  only  a 
single  interval  is  dissonant,  and  the  rest  are  consonant.  These  are  most  teadily 
formed  by  uniting  two  consonant  triads  which  have  two  tones  in  common.  In  this 
case  the  tones  which  are  not  in  common  to  the  two  chords  are  dissonant  to  each  ^ 
other,  and  the  rest  are  consonant,  so  that  the  dissonance  is  comparatively  un- 
observed t  amid  the  mass  of  consonances.    Thus  the  triads 

c  +  ^i  -gr 

on  being  fused  give  the  tetrad 

c  +  Bi—g  +  bi 

in  which  the  major  Seventh  c.&i  is  a  dissonant  interval  and  the  other  intervals 
are  consonant,  as  the  annexed  scheme  shews : — 

*  [Which    are    made    spontaneously    in  denariom,  App.  XX.  sect.  E.  art.  18. — TranS' 

equally  tempered  intonation,  where  all  three  latorj] 

chords  are  absolutely  identical,  bat  would  other-  f  [To  my  sensation  the  dissonant  tones 

wise  require  an  entire  sacrifice  of  the  feeling  utterly  destroy  the  consonance. — Translator.^ 
of  tonality.    Follow  these  chords  on  the  Duo- 


Digitized  by 


^.joogle 


342  CHORDS  OP  THE   SEVENTH.  pabt  in. 

[In  cents : 
c  702  gr,  ei  702  bi 
C  386  El  316  G  386  Bi 
c  1088  6 J.] 

¥ 

This  position  of  the  chord  of  the  Seventh,  deduced  from  the  closest  positions  of 
the  triads,  is  regarded  as  fundamental  or  primary.  The  intervals  between  the 
individual  tones  appear  as  Thirds,  and  when  chords  of  the  Seventh  are  formed 
from  the  consonant  triads  of  the  scale,  these  Thirds  will  be  alternately  major  and 
minor,  because  consonant  triads  always  unite  a  major  with  a  minor  Third.  Haupt- 
mann  calls  these  chords  of  the  Seventh  which  occur  spontaneously  in  the  natnial 
f  series  of  Thirds  of  a  key 

the  chords  of  the  direct  system  or  simply  direct  chords.  There  are  two  kinds  of 
these  chords.  In  one  a  minor  Third  lies  between  two  major  Thirds,  as  in  the 
tetrad  c+61—  gf+bi  already  cited,  and  similarly  in  /  +  ai—  c'+  e/  in  G  major, 
and  il*[?H-c— c*b+gr  in  0  minor.  In  the  other  a  major  Third  lies  between  two 
minor  Thirds,  as  in 

%  [In  cents : 

1'       '       ^  a  702  e'l,  c'  702  g' 

aj-c'  +  £jV-i/'  ^1  316  G  386  E,  316  G 

V yK.,^^y — ^  ai  loiS  g\] 

i      i       i 

f 
^  and  similarly  in  ei  —  ^  +  6,  —  dj  in  C  major  and  /— a*|?+c— «*l>  in  C  minor.     In 
this  second  species  the  dissonance  is  a  minor  Seventh,  f,  [roughness  25,  p.  332, 
Table,  No.  36,  cents  1018,]  which  is  much  milder  than  the  major  Seventh,  '^  [ibid. 
No.  37,  cents  1088,  roughness  42]. 

B.  Chords  of  the  Seventh  formed  of  Dissonant  Triads, 

Other  chords  of  the  Seventh  may  be  formed  from  the  dissonant  triads  of  the 
key,  each  united  with  one  consonant  triad,  and  also  from  the  two  dissonant  triads 
themselves.     By  thus  uniting  the  limiting  tones  of  the  series  of  chords  in  the  key, 

major:  c+Ci   ^g+bi—  d  \  f+a^   —c 

and  minor:   c— e*b+gf-f  6,— d  |  /— a*l>+c 

we  obtain  the  following  Chords  of  the  Seventh  in  the  reverted  system^  or  iiidirect 
f  tetrads : 

[In  cents : 
1)  1^  i)        g  ^02  d\  bi  610  f 

J' ' ^  G  386  J5,  316  D  294  F 

gJtb,-d'  \  f  17  996  /] 


f       f 


33 


V- 
2)  I  [2)       d  680  a,,/ 702  c' 

JO. -_  D  2942^386^1,  316  C 

d  996  c'] 


d  I  f  -^-a^-c' 


Digitized  by 


Google 


CHAP.  XVII.  CHORDS  OP  THE  SEVENTH. 

3)  f  [3)       d6joa%f'jo2e' 

^.^^~"       ^  D  294  F  316  A^\}  386  G 

d  I  f-a^]}+&  dgg6&] 

7T 


343 


31       e         J 


V- 

4)  ^  [4)        6,  610/,  d  680  a, 


fl4  . 


ty— " ^  Bi  3i6Z)294jP386il, 


6  3  3         A 


5)  n  [5)        b,  610  f,d  610  a'\}  f 

B,  3i6D294F3i6il»t> 
bi  926  a^W 


The  Sevenths  of  these  chords  all  come  pretty  near  to  the  natural  Seventh  ^ 
[cents  969],  and  are  all  smaller  than  the  Sevenths  in  the  chords  of  the  Seventh 
formed  from  two  consonant  triads  [cents  1088  and  1018].  The  principal  disso- 
nances in  these  chords  are  the  false  and  imperfect  Fifths  bi...f,  d...aif  and  d,..a^\}, 
that  is,  the  intervals  $|  and  ^  [p.  332,  Table,  Nos.  21  and  23,  cents  610  and  680, 
roughnesses  28  and  44].  Hence  the  first  three  of  these  chords  of  the  Seventh, 
g  +  bi  —  d  \  ff  d  I  /  +  ^1  —  c,  and  d  \  f"-a^\}  +  c,  each  of  which  contains  only  one 
of  these  imperfect  Fifths,  are  less  harshly  dissonant  than  the  two  last,  each  off 
-which  contains  two  of  them.  Such  of  these  chords  as  contain  a  major  triad, 
namely — 

g   +  fei  —  d   I  /     and     d   |  /  +  ^i  —  c 

I !  I I 

are  about  equal  in  dissonance  to  the  milder  chords  of  the  Seventh  in  the  direct 
system,  which  contains  the  larger  and  rougher  kind  of  Sevenths,  and,  at  the  same 
time,  only  perfect  Fifths,  viz. : 


ai—c  +  Bi—g    and    Bi  ^  g  +  bi  —  d 

The  chord  of  the  dominant  Seventh  g  +  bi—  d^  \  f  can  be  even  rendered 
much  milder  by  lowering  its  /  to//.     The  interval  g..»fi    corresponds  to  the 
ratio  VW  [cents  974],  which  is  very  nearly  equal  to  J  [cents  969],  being  =  Jx  f 
^i%  [cents  969  +  5],  or  approximately  i  x|^^.     Hence  the  chord  gf  +  61— ^  |  /i 
is  on  the  verge  of  consonance.* 

But  the  chord  of  the  Seventh  which  contains  a  fialse  Fifth  and  a  minor  triad, 
namely  No.  3  above,  or 

is  about  as  rough  as  the  tetrads  of  the  direct  system  containing  a  major  Seventh, 
namely — 


*  [That  is,  allowing  g  h^d^f  or  c  e^g'hb  to  be  satisfactory  imitation — especially  by  ears  nn- 

consonant.    In  the  53  division  the  player  uses  accustomed  to  the  true  interval,  because  it  is  so 

44  degrees  »  996  cents  for  ^.. ./,  and  43  degrees  much  superior  to  the  former  of  44  degrees  =>  996 

■1974  cents  for  g...7,  and  the  latter  is  found  a  oent8.-*TnirM2ator.] 


Digitized  by  VjOOQlC 


344 


CHOBDS  OF  THE  SEVENTH. 


PABT  m. 


It  is  carious  that  the  first  of  these  three  tetrads  oontains  exactly  the  same  intervals 
as  the  chord  of  the  dominant  Seventh  itself,  g  +  b^  —  d  \  f,  only  in  inverse  order, 

thus — 


U 


In  the  first  the  consonant  portion  is  a  minor  triad,  and  this  makes  it  decidedly 
harsher  than  the  second  where  the  consonant  portion  is  a  major  triad. 

Here  also  the  difference  of  harshness  depends  on  the  nature  of  the  combina- 
tional tones,  of  which  those  generated  by  the  closer  intervals  are  most  distinctly 
heard.     These  are 

for    ^'  +  6/-(i"  I  /'    and  for    d"  \  /'-a»"t>  +  c'" 


f 


G     G      Ai 


Ai     d^\}       a^\} 


Hence  one  combinational  tone  in  the  first  chord,  and  two  in  the  second,  are  un- 
suitable to  the  chord.  " 

The  harshest  chords  of  the  Seventh  are  those  which  each  contain  two  false 
Fifths,. namely,  No.  ^orbi—d]  Z+a/  and  No.  5  or  &i-d'  |  /-a*'t>-  But  the 
first  of  these  can  be  made  much  milder  by  a  slight  change  in  its  intonation.  Thus 
bi  —  d...fi'...a'  contains  tones  which  all  belong  to  the  compound  tone  of  G,,  and 
these  sound  tolerably  well  together.* 

The  chords  of  the  reverted  system  play  an  important  part  in  modulations,  by 
serving  to  mark  the  key  precisely.  The  most  decisive  in  its  action  is  the  chord  of 
the  Seventh  on  the  dominant  of  the  key,  that  is  the  chord  g  +  bi  —  d  |  /  for  the 
tonic  C.  We  saw  (p.  341 5)  that  the  diminished  triad  bi  —  d  \  f  could  be  adapted 
by  slight  changes  in  its  intonation  to  the  keys  of 

IT  G  major,  C  minor,  Ai  minor,  i^,  J  minor,  and  E^\}  minor. 

Of  these  only  the  two  first  contain  the  tone  G,  so  that  the  chord  g  +  bi  —  d  \  f 
can  belong  to  no  tonic  but  G. 

The  imperfect  minor  triad  [or  chord  of  the  added  Sixth]  d  \  /+  ^i,  which, 
when  the  intonation  is  correct,  belongs  only  to  the  key  of  G  major,  admits  of  being 
confused  [and  is  in  equal  temperament  always  identified]  with  di  — /+  ai,  which 


*  [This  is  only  to  be  taken  as  an  approxi- 
mative statement,  grounded  on  the  assumption 
that  the  interval  g  to  //  is  correctly  J,  in  which 
case  the  primes  of  the  tones  6,,  d,/,',  o' are 
the  5th,  6th,  7th,  9th  partials  of  G.  This 
chord  in  its  true  formation  is  used  on  Mr. 
Poole*8  double  or  dichordal  scale  F  G  A''Bb 
cdCify  the  two  chords  being  F  :  A^  :  c  =  4.l  $  : 
6,  and  C  :  ^,  :  G  :  '-Bb  :  d  =  4  :  5  :  6  :  7  :  9. 
^  In  the  text  it  is,  in  point  of  fact,  proposed  to 
use  B^ b  in  the  chord  C  Ei  G  B^b  d^  as  an  imi- 
tation of  Mr.  Poole's  natural  chord,  which 
"^'ould  be  still  closer  than  C  E  G  A^  d^  with 
the  extreme  sharp  Sixth  in  place  of  the 
natural  Seventh.  In  fact,  C  :  ^Bb  =  969  cents, 
C  :  B,b  =  974  cents,  and  C  :  A^t  «976  cents. 

To  test  the  effect  of  septimal  intonation  I 
had  an  instrument  tuned  to  give  the  chords — 

BbdJ,FA,C,  CE.G'Bbd,  GB^d,DF'A^ 
perfectly  (of  which  the  second,  third,  and 
fourth  occur  on  the  Harmonical).  The  effect 
of  the  third  of  these  chords  far  surpasses  my 
expectations,  and  it  is  beyond  comparison 
better  than  the  usual  chord  of  the  Ninth  with 
Bb  d  in  place  of  ^Bbd  (for  which  on  the  Har- 
monical gb^dfa^  can  be  played).  The  chord 
of  the  Bubminor  Seventh  and  its  inversions 

CE^Q 'Bb.  E^G^Bbc,  G "Bb  c Cp  »Bb  ce,g 


are  all  decidedly  superior  to  the  chord  of  the 
dominant  Seventh,  with  Bb  in  place  of  '£b, 
and  its  inversions  (which  on  the  Harmonical 
must  be  tried  as  ^  &,  (2  /  and  its  inversions). 
The  septimal  minor  triad  G  'Bb  <2  is  far  snpe- 
rior  to  the  Pythagorean  minor  triad  D  P  A 
(not  on  the  Harmonical),  or  the  false  minor 
triad  D  F  A^^  and  is  not  far  inferior  to  the 
true  minor  triad  Bj  F  A^  or  D  F^  A  (on  the 
Harmonical  compare  g^bbd'  with  gb^bd).  The 
septimal  diminished  triad  ^^  G  'Bb  approaches 
consonance  much  more  nearly  than  the  nsaal 
diminished  triad  E^  G  Bb  (play  6,  d/  on  the 
Harmonical).  Though  Poolers  ascending  scale 
makes  too  great  a  gap  between  'Bb  and  c,  yet 
by  using  'Bb  B  as  alternative  tones  with  Bb 
Bp  ascending  with  the  sharper  and  descending 
with  the  flatter  forms,  we  obtain  the  perfectly 
melodious  scales  of 

F  G  A^Bbc  d  e^f  and  f  e^d^c  'Bb  A^QF 

'of  which  the  first,  being  the  ordinary  scale  of 
i^  major,  does  not  exist  on  the  Harmonical, 
which  has  no  Bb,  but  the  second  can  be  played 
upon  it).  These  facts  shew  the  acoustic  possi- 
bility of  a  septimal  theory  of  harmony,  which 
would  include  the  tertian,  or  ordinary  harmony 
of  just  intonation. — Translator>1 


^' 


Digitized  by  VjOOQIC 


CHAP.  XVII.  CHOKDS  OF  THE   SEVENTH.  345 

belongs  to  the  keys  of  Ai  minor,  J^  major,  and  £J>  major.  This  confusion  is  not 
entirely  obviated  by  adding  the  tone  c,  and  the  consequence  is  the  chord  of  the  Seventh 
d  I  /  +  a,  —  c  is  usually  employed  only  in  alternation  with  the  chord  of  the 
dominant  Seventh  in  the  cadence,  where  it  distinguishes  G  major  from  C  minor.* 
But  the  addition  of  the  tone  b^  to  the  triad  d  |  /  +  ^i  [as  fej  —  ei  \  f  '\-  a{]  is 
characteristic,  because  this  last  can  at  most  be  confused  with  5|  |  ^1  —  /  +  ai, 
which  belongs  to  A^  minor.  The  chord  h]  —  d  \  f  -^  a^,  however,  sounds  com- 
paratively harsh  in  every  position  for  which  a,  is  not  the  highest  note,  and  hence 
its  application  is  very  limited.  It  is  often  united  with  the  chord  of  the  dominant 
Seventh  as  a  chord  of  the  Ninth,  thus  g  +  b^  —  d'  |  /  +  a/,  in  which  g  and  ai' 
must  remain  the  extreme  tones.     More  upon  this  hereafter. 

In  the  key  of  C  minor,  the  triad  d  \  f  ^  cl^\}  would,  in  just  intonation,  be 
characteristic,  but  yet  it  is  easily  confused  with  other  chords.     Thus 

H 
d  \  f  —  a^\}  [in  cents  d  294/316  a^\}]  belongs  to  G  minor 

^1  ~  /  I  a|?  [in  cents  d  316  /  294  a\}]  to  E\}  major  and  E\}  minor 

6  3  2 

S         ST 

d  — /^..{/ij  [in  cents  d  316/*  274  <7iJ]  to  A  minor 
d^..,  e,J  -gfj  [in  cents  d^  274  eijf  316  ^  to  Fjj^  minor. 

75  r. 

The  addition  of  the  tone  G  in  the  first  chord  of  the  Seventh  above,  thus 
^  I  /  -  ct*|?  4-  c,  would  decisively  exclude  the  key  i^  minor  only,  and  the  addition 
of  the  tone  ^i  (which  in  tempered  intonation  is  confused  with  b  or  c^\yj  would  also 
readily  be  adapted  to  all  the  above  keys.  Thus  altered  it  becomes  the  chord  61  —  IT 
d  I  f  —  a^\},  and  is  called  th^  chord  of  the  diminished  Seventh,  which  on  keyed 
instruments  appears  as  a  series  of  minor  Thirds.  In  reality  a  Pythagorean  minor 
Third  or  else  an  acute  augmented  Second  separates  the  normal  minor  Thirds,  thus : 

bi-d   \  f-^a'\)  .  b.-d   I  /-a»t>...^^i 

[In  cents:       316294  316     274  316  294316     274] 

Since  the  three  intervals  |,  -|f,  and  J-{  [cents  316,  294,  274]  differ  but  very 
slightly  [by  20,  22,  42  cents  respectively],  they  are  readily  confused,t  and  we  obtain 
the  following,  nearly  identical,  series  of  tones : 

bi— d  I  /  —  a*l>...Z>i  [in  cents  6,  316  ^  294/316  a*|:>  2746,]  in  C  minor 

6        33  6  75 

b   1   d"  f  ...  gi$  ^  b  \m  cents  b  294  d  316  f  274  gjj^  316  b]inA  minor 
b  —  d^ ...  61J  —  (/jf  I  5  [in  cents  b  316  d^  274  ei#  316  gf#  294  6]  in  i^  minor 

6  7  5  6  3  2 

c^l>  ...  ^1—  /  I   a\}  -  c*t>  [in  cents  c^\}  274  di  316/294  a\}  316  c^\}]  in  E\}  minor. 

75  6         3  2  6 

*  [This  arises  entirely  from  temperament,  hence  in  all  written  music  they  are  treated  as 

which  identifies  the  two  chords  d  |/+a,  — c,  identical.     The  four    following  forms  of  the 

and  rf,  -/+  ai—c.    Listen  to  the  difiference  on  chord  (of  which  only  the  first  can  be  played  on 

the  Harmonioal.— TmrwZator.]  the  Harmonical)  are  struck  with  absolutely  the 

t  [It  is  quite  impossible  to  confuse  them  same  digitals  on  a  pianoforte.    Trace  them  on 

in  the  just  intonation  of  any  harmonic  inter-  the  Duodenarium,  App.  XX.  sect.  E.  art.  18. — 


▼als,  but    they  are    absolutely  identified  in      Translator.} 
equally  tempered  intonation  as  300  cents,  and 


Digitized  by 


Google 


S46  CHOEDS  OF  THE   SEVENTH.  part  m. 

These  chords  of  the  diminished  Seventh  do  not  form  so  sharp  a  contrast  with 
the  consonances  in  the  minor  mode,  as  the  corresponding  chord  does  in  the  major 
mode,  although  if  the  intonation  is  just  the  dissonance  is  always  extremely  harsh 
and  cutting.*  When  they  are  followed  by  the  triad  of  the  tonic,  the  two  chords 
together  contain  all  the  tones  of  the  key,  and  hence  completely  characterise  it. 
The  chief  use  of  the  chord  of  the  diminished  Seventh  is  due  to  its  variability, 
which  readily  leads  the  harmony  into  new  keys.  By  merely  subjoining  the  minor 
chords  of  i^.  A,  C  or  ^  the  new  key  will  be  completely  established.  It  is  readily 
seen  that  this  series  of  keys  itself  forms  a  chord  of  the  diminished  Seventh,  the 
tones  of  which  lie  a  Semitone  higher  than  those  of  the  given  chord.  This  gives  a 
simple  means  of  recollecting  them.f 

The  comprehension  of  the  whole  of  a  key  by  these  chords  is  of  special  impor- 
tance in  the  cadence  at  the  end  of  a  composition  or  of  one  of  its  principal  sections. 

%  For  this  purpose  we  have  also  to  determine  what  fundamental  primary  tones  can 
be  represented  by  these  chords  of  the  Seventh. 

It  is  clear  that  a  single  musical  tone  can  never  be  more  than  imperfectly 
represented  by  the  tones  of  a  dissonant  chord.  But  as  a  general  rule  some  of 
these  tones  can  be  accepted  as  the  constituents  of  a  musical  tone.  This  gives  rise 
to  a  practically  important  difference  between  the  different  tones  of  such  a  chord. 
Those  tones  which  can  be  considered  as  the  elements  of  a  compound  tone,  form  a  com- 
pact, well-defined  mass  of  tone.  Any  one  or  two  other  tones  in  the  chord,  which  do 
not  belong  to  this  mass  of  tone,  have  the  appearance  of  unconnected  tones,  acci- 
dentally intruding.  The  latter  are  called  by  musicians  the  dissonances  or  the  dis- 
sonant  notes  of  the  chord.  Considered  independently,  of  course,  either  tone  in  a 
dissonant  interval  is  equally  dissonant  in  respect  to  the  other,  and  if  there  were  only 
two  tones  it  would  be  absurd  to  call  one  of  them  only  the  dissonant  tone.  In 
the  Seventh  c.bi,  cis  dissonant  in  respect  to  &i,  and  bi  in  respect  to  c.    In  the 

%  chord  c  +  ^1  —  gf  +  61  the  notes  c  -h  61  —  gf  form  a  single  mass  of  tone  corresponding 
to  the  compound  tone  of  c,  and  bi  is  an  unconnected  tone  sounding  at  the  same 
time.    Hence  the  three  tones  c  +  ^1  —  ^have  an  independent  steadiness  and  compact- 

*  [As  the  ratios  4  :  5  :  6  :  7  are  the  justifi-  four  keys  into  which  a  slight  alteration  of  the 

cation  of  the  chord  of  ihe  dominant  Seventh  pitches  of  the  notes  in  the  chord  of  the  dimi- 

4  :  5  :  6  :  7j,  so  the  ratios  10  :  12  :  14  :  17  are  nished  Seventh  will  make  it  fit,  are  FJl,  A^  C, 

the  justification  of  the  chord  of  the  diminished  E\}.   These  notes,  however,  do  not  form  a  chord 

Seventh  10  :  12  :  14J  :  17^  taking  the  ratios  with  the  same  intervals,  but  Ft  294  A  294  C 

of  No.  5,  p.  343&,  and  commencing  with  10.  294 ^b, that  is  a  succession  of  Pythagorean  minor 

That  is,  e"3i6  ^'267  '6"b  336  *'d'"b,  which  Thirds,  the  result  of  which  is  simply  hideooA. 

can  be  played  on  the  Harmonical,  is  the  just  It  is  only  in  equally  tempered  intonation  in 

chord  of  the  diminished  Seventh,  for  which  which  the  four  forms  above  given  of  the  chord 

the  form  of  ordinary  just  intonation  is  e''3i6  of  the  diminished  Seventh  agree  absolutely  in 

g^'2f)^  &''b3i6  d'b,  which  must  be  played  as  sound,  though  they  di£Fer  in  writing,  because 

^^316  2)/294  /'316  a^"\>  on  the  Harmonical,  signs  originally  intended  for  other  tempera- 

an  intensely  harsh  chord,  for  which  is  played  ments  (as  the  Pythagorean,  meantone,  or  other 

in  equal  temperament  /300  6^300  /'300  a"b.  which  distinguished  Cjf  and  Db,  but  did  not 

IT  Observe  that  the  dimini&ed  Seventh  10  :  17  distinguish  the  conmia)  have  continued  in  use, 

has  919  cents,  the    diminished  Seventh  of  with  confounded  meanings.    This  is  precisely 

ordinary  just    intonation    10  :  17^  has  926  the  same  as  in  ordinary  English    speUing, 

cents,  7  too  sharp ;  while  in  equally  tempered  where  combinations  of  letters  originally  repre- 

intonation  it  is  only  900  cents  or  19  too  flat.  senting  very  different  sounds,  are  now  con- 

And    the    tempered    major   Sixth   is    repre-  fused,  as  I  have  demonstrated  historically  in 

sented  by  the  same    interval  of  900  cents,  my  Early  English  Pronunciation.    In  equally 

which  is  16  cents  too  sharp.    It  is  remarkable  tempered  intonation  the  roots  ft  300  a  300  c 

that  any  sense  of  interval  or  tonality  survives  300  ^b  do  also  form  a  chord  of  the  diminished 

these  confusions.    Of  course  the  introduction  Seventh.    But  this  does  not  end  the  confusion, 

of  the  17th  harmonic  into  the  scale  is  a  sheer  for  the  key  oift  may  betaken  as  that  of  grb,  of 

impossibility.    The  chord  10  :  12  :  14J  :  17^  a  as  that  of  &bb,  c  as  that  of  &S  ,  eb  as  that  of 

is  simple  noise.    The  chord  10  :  12  :  14  :  17  dt  ,and  these  four  roots,  ab,  &b  \>^ht^dt ,  being 

which  I  have  tried  on  Appunn's  tonometer  in  played  with  the  same  digitals  represent  the 

its  inversions,  is  a  comparatively  smooth  dis-  same  chord,  but  the  four  keys  are  now  totally 

cord  superior  to  the  tempered  form.    But  the  unrelated.    What  then  becomes  of  the  feeling 

chord  is  really  due  to  tempered  intonation  of  tonality?  and  how  are  we  to  feel  the  right 

only.    For  further  notes  on  this  chord  see  amid  this  mass  of  wrong,  as  Sir  George  Mac- 

App.  XX.  sect.  E.  art.  23,  and  sect.  F.  towards  farren  says  we  can,  and  as  I  must  thereforo 

end  of  No.  7.— SVansIator.]  suppose  he  himself  hjM  sacceeded  in  doing  ? — 

t  [It  is  correctly  stated  in  the  text  that  the  Translator,']      Digitized  by  ^OOQIC 


CHAP,  XVII.    DISSONANT  NOTES  IN  CHOBDS  OF  THE  SEVENTH.     347 

ness  of  their  own.  But  the  unsupported  solitary  Seventh  bi  has  to  stand  against 
the  preponderance  of  the  other  tones,  and  it  could  not  do  so  either  when  executed 
by  a  singer,  or  heard  by  a  listener,  unless  the  melodic  progression  were  kept  very 
simple  and  readily  intelligible.  Consequently  particular  rules  have  to  be  observed 
for  the  progression  of  the  part  which  produces  this  note,  whereas  the  introduction 
of  c,  which  is  sufficiently  justified  by  the  chord  itself,  is  perfectly  free  and  unfettered. 
Musicians  indicate  this  practical  difference  in  the  laws  of  progression  of  parts  by 
terming  by  alone  the  dissonant  note  of  this  chord;  and  although  the  expression 
is  not  a  very  happy  one,  we  can  have  no  hesitation  in  retaining  it,  after  its  real 
meaning  has  been  thus  explained. 

We  now  proceed  to  examine  each  of  the  previous  chords  of  the  Seventh  with  a 
view  to  determine  what  compound  musical  tone  they  represent,  and  which  are  their 
dissonant  tones. 

1.  The  chard  of  the  dominant  Seventh,  g  +  bi—d\f,  contains  three  tones f 
belonging  to  the  compound  tone  of  G,  namely^,  ^j,  and  (2,  and  the  Seventh  /  is  the 
dissonant  tone.  But  we  must  observe  that  the  minor  Seventh  ^ . .  /  [or  *^  =  |^  x  J,  or 
cents  996  =  969  +  27]  approaches  so  near  to  the  ratio  i  [cents  969]  which  would  be 
almost  exactly  represented  by  ^ . . ./, ,  [cents  974] ,  that  /  may  in  any  case  pass  as  the 
seventh  partial  tone  of  the  compound  G*  Singers  probably  often  exchange  the/ 
of  the  chord  of  the  dominant  Seventh  for/i,  t  partly  because  it  usually  passes  into  e|, 
partly  because  they  thus  diminish  the  harshness  of  the  dissonance.  This  can  be 
easily  done  when  the  pitch  of/  is  not  determined  in  the  preceding  chord  by  some 
near  relationship.  Thus  if  the  consonant  chord  g-\'bi—d  had  already  been  struck 
and  then  /  were  added,  it  would  readily  fall  into/i,  [^h.Q,t  is  ^/j  because/  is  to  itself 
unrelated  to  g,  b^j  or  d.t  Hence,  although  the  chord  of  the  dominant  Seventh 
is  dissonant,  its  dissonant  tone  so  nearly  corresponds  to  the  corresponding  partial 
tone  in  the  compound  tone  of  the  dominant,  that  the  whole  chord  may  be  very  well 
regarded  as  a  representative  of  that  compound.  For  this  reason,  doubtless,  the  f 
Seventh  of  this  chord  has  been  set  free  from  many  obHgations  in  the  progression 
of  parts  to  which  dissonant  Sevenths  are  otherwise  subjected.  Thus  it  is  allowed 
to  be  introduced  freely  without  preparation,  which  is  not  the  case  for  the  other 
Sevenths.  In  modem  compositions  (as  E.  Wagner's)  the  chord  of  the  dominant 
Seventh  not  unfrequently  occurs  as  the  concluding  chord  of  a  subordinate  section 
of  a  piece  of  music. 

The  chord  of  the  dominant  Seventh  consequently  plays  the  second  most  impor- 
tant part  in  modem  music,  standing  next  to  the  tonic.  It  exactly  defines  the  key» 
more  exactly  than  the  simple  triad  g  +  b^—d,  01  than  the  diminished  triad  bi—d  |  /. 
As  a  dissonant  chord  it  urgently  requires  to  be  resolved  on  to  the  tonic  chord, 
which  the  simple  dominant  triad  does  not.  And  finally  its  harmoniousness  is  so 
extremely  little  obscured,  that  it  is  the  softest  of  all  dissonant  chords.§  Hence  we 
could  scarcely  do  without  it  in  modem  music.  This  chord  appears  to  have  been 
discovered  in  the  beginning  of  the  seventeenth  century  by  Monteverde.  f 

2 .  The  chord  of  the  Seventh  upon  the  Second  of  a  major  scale,  d  \  /+  a,  —  c,  has 
three  tones,/,  a^,  c,  which  belong  to  the  compound  tone  of  F.  When  the  intona- 
tion is  just,  d  is  dissonant  with  each  of  the  three  tones  of  this  chord,  and  hence  must 

♦  [It  has,  however,  a  very  different  effect  on  such  points)  that  /,  is  very  remote  indeed 

on  the  ear. — Translator.']  from  g. — Translator.] 

t  [Here  /,  must  be  considered  as  the  repre-  §  [As  we  hear  it  only  in  tempered  music  as  a 

sentative  of  y.     Singers  would  not  naturally  rule,  with  the  harsh  major  Third,  which  makes 

take  such  a  strange  artificial  approximation  as  the  major  triad  almost  dissonant,  the  addition 

/,,   unless  led  by  an  instrument.    Unaccom-  of  the  dominant  Seventh  increases  the  harsh- 

panied  singers  could  only  choose  between  /  and  ness  surprisingly  little.    But  in  just  intonation 

y,  and  singers  of  unaccompanied  melodies  are  g  b^df  is  markedly  harsher  than  g  b^  d  %  as 

said  often  to  choose  '/  when  descending  to  e.  I  have  often  had  occasion  to  observe  in  Ap- 

What  is  the  custom  in  unaccompanied  choirs,  punn's  tonometer,  where  g  b^  d  can  be  left 

which  have  not  been  trained  to  give  /,  has,  so  sounding,  and  /  suddenly  transformed  to  */  and 

far  as  I  know,  not  bepn  recorded. — Translator.]  conversely.    On  the  Harmonical  we  must  oom- 

%  [And  '/  is,  but/i  again  is  not.    It  will  be  pare  g  b^d  f  with  ce^g  ^5b,  and  that  in  aU 

seen  by  the  Duodenarium  (App.  XX.  sect.  E.  their  inversions  and  positions. — Translator,] 
art.  18)  (which  should  be  constantly  consulted  *  Digitized  by  V^OOQIC 


348        DISSONANT  NOTES  IN  CHOKDS  OP  THE  SEVENTH,    part  in. 

be  regarded  as  the  dissonant  note.  This  would  make  the  fundamental  position  of 
this  chord  to  be  that  which  Eameau  assigned,  making/the  root,  thus  :/+  ^i  —  c  ...fZ, 
which  is  a  position  of  the  Sixth  and  Fifth,  and  the  chord  is  called  by  Eameau  the 
cJiord  of  the  great  Sixth  [grande  Sixte,  in  English  *  added  Sixth'].  This  is  the 
position  in  which  the  chord  usually  appears  in  the  final  cadence  of  C  major.  Its 
meaning  and  its  relation  to  the  key  is  more  certain  than  that  of  the  false  minor 
chord  d  \  f+a^^  mentioned  on  p.  340a,  which  as  executed  by  a  singer  or  heard  by 
a  listener  is  readily  apt  to  be  confused  with  di  —f  +  ai  in  the  key  of  Ai  minor. 
By  changing  d  |  /+«!  into  d^  — /  +  ai  we  obtain  a  minor  chord,  to  which  there 
will  be  a  great  attraction  when  the  relation  of  ^  to  ^  is  not  made  very  distinct. 
But  if  we  were  to  change  d  into  di  in  the  chord  d  \  /H-ai— c,  thus  producing 
di  — /+  «!  —  c,  although  di  would  be  consonant  with  /and  ai  it  would  not  be  so 
with  c ;  on  the  contrary,  the  dissonance  dy-.x'  [p.  332,  No.  36,  cents  1018,  rough- 
f  ness  25]  is  much  harsher  than  d...c\  [ibid..  No.  34,  cents  996,  roughness  23, 
much  the  same  as  the  other] ,  and,  after  all,  it  would  be  only  the  tone  ai  which 
would  enter  into  the  compound  tone  of  d^,  so  that,  notwithstanding  this  change,  /, 
which  contains  three  tones  of  the  chord  in  its  own  compound  tone,  would  predomi- 
nate over  d,,  which  has  only  two.  In  accordance  with  this  view,  I  find  the  chord 
/-h  ^i  —  c...{?  when  used  on  the  justly-intoned  harmonium,  as  subdominant  of  C 
major,  produces  a  better  effect  than/+  ai—c.di. 

3.  The  corresponding  chord  of  the  Seventh  on  the  Second  of  the  minor  scale^ 
d  \f—  a^\}  -h  c,  has  only  one  tone,  c,  which  can  be  regarded  as  a  constituent  of  the 
compound  tone  of  either  /  or  a^\}.  But  since  c  is  the  third  partial  of  /  and  only 
the  fifth  partial  of  a*[7,  /  as  a  rule  predominates,  and  the  chord  must  be  regarded 
as  a  subdominant  chord  /—  a^|?  +  c  with  the  addition  of  dissonant  d.  There  is 
still  less  inducement  to  change  d  into  di  in  this  case  than  in  the  last. 

4.  The  chord  of  the  Seventh  on  the  Seventh  of  the  major  scale,  61  —  d  |  /+  ^i, 
^  contains  two  tones,  bi  and  d,  belonging  to  the  dominant  g,  and  two  others,  /,  and 

ai ,  belonging  to  the  subdominant  /.  Hence  the  chord  splits  into  two  equally  im- 
portant halves.  But  we  must  observe  that  the  two  tones  /  and  Ay  approach  very 
closely  to  the  two  next  partial  tones  of  the  compound  tone  of  (7.  The  partials  of 
this  compound  tone  from  the  fourth  onwards  may  be  written — 

g-]-bi-d..,fi.,.g...a 
4    5      6    7*    8     9 

Hence  the  chord  of  the  Ninth  g  +  b^—d  \  f+a^  may  represent  the  compound  tone 
of  the  dominant  g,  provided  that  the  similarity  be  kept  clear  by  the  position  of  the 
tones,  g  being  the  lowest  and  a^  the  highest ;  it  is  also  best  not  to  let  /  [standing 
for  y]  fall  too  low.  Since  a  is  the  ninth  partial  tone  of  the  compound  g,  which  is 
very  weak  in  all  usual  quahties  of  tone,  and  is  often  inaudible,  and  since  there 
is  the  interval  of  a  comma  between  a  and  ax ,  and  also  between  /j  and  /  [but  y  and  / 
%  differ  by  27  cents],  care  must  be  taken  to  render  the  resemblance  of  the  chord  of 
the  Ninth  to  the  compound  tone  of  g,  as  strong  as  possible,  by  adopting  the  device 
of  keeping  a,  uppermost,  and  then  tiie  use  of  /,  a,,  for  /i,  a,  [meaning  y  a]  wiU 
not  be  very  striking.  In  this  case  /  and  a^  must  be  considered  as  the  cUssonant 
notes  of  the  chord  of  the  Ninth  g  +  bi—d  \  f+ai,  because  although  they  are  very 
nearly  the  same,  they  are  not  quite  the  same,  as  the  partial  tones  of  G.  No  pre- 
paration is  necessary  for  the  introduction  of  a^  into  the  chord,  for  the  same  reasons 
that  /  is  allowed  to  be  introduced  into  the  chord  of  the  dominant  Seventh, 
g+bi—d  I  /  without  preparation.  Lastly,  some  of  the  tones  of  the  pentad  chord 
of  the  Ninth  may  be  omitted,  to  reduce  it  to  four  parts ;  for  example,  its  Fifth,  as 
in  g  +  bi  ..f+ai,  or  its  root,  as  in  bi^d  \  /+«!.  If  only  the  order  of  the  tones  is 
preserved  as  much  as  possible,  and  especially  the  a^  kept  uppermost,  the  chord  will 
always  be  recognised  as  a  representative  of  the  compound  tone  of  G. 

*  (That  is,  supposing  /i  to  be  used  for  so  that  the  above  chord  represents  gbid^fg  a. 
y,  as  abready  explained,  see  p.  347<J,  note  f,      — Trcmslaior.^ 

Digitized  by  V^OOQIC 


CHAP.  XVII.    DISSONANT  NOTES  IN  CHOKDS  OP  THE  SEVENTH.     349 

This  seems  to  me  the  simple  reason  why  musicians  find  it  desirable  to  make  ai 
the  highest  tone  in  the  chord  bi—d\  /H-ap  Hauptmann,  indeed,  gives  this  as  a 
rule  without  exception,  and  assigns  rather  an  artificial  reason  for  it.  The  ambiguity 
of  the  chord  will  thus  be  obviated  as  far  as  possible,  and  it  receives  a  clearly  in- 
telhgible  relation  to  the  dominant  of  the  key  of  C  major,  whereas  in  other  positions 
of  the  same  chord  there  would  be  too  great  a  .chance  of  confusing  it  with  the  sub- 
dominant  of  A I  minor.*  When  the  intonation  is  j  ust ,  the  chord  gr  +  6 ,  —  6? . .  ./i . . .  a,f 
which  consists  (very  nearly  indeed)  of  the  partial  tones  of  the  compound  tone  of  g, 
sounds  very  soft,  and  but  slightly  dissonant ;  the  chord  of  the  Ninth  in  the  key 
of  C  major,  g-^bi-^d'  l/'+a/,  and  the  chord  of  the  Seventh  in  the  position 
hi—d'  IZ+a/,  sound  somewhat  rougher,  on  account  of  the  Pythagorean  Third 
d'  I/,  and  the  imperfect  Fifth  ^'...a/,  but  they  are  not  very  harsh.  If,  however, 
a/  is  taken  in  a  lower  position,  they  become  veiy  rough  indeed. 

The  chord  of  the  Seventh  hi—d  [/+«!  and  the  following  triad  c-he^—g,  as  ^ 
already  observed,  contain  all  the  tones  in  the  key  of  C  major,  and  hence  this 
chordal  succession  is  extremely  well  adapted  for  a  brief  and  complete  characterisa- 
tion of  the  key. 

5.  The  chord  of  the  diminished  Seventh ,  bi—d\  f—a^\},  and  the  minor  chord 
c  —  e^\}+g,  have  the  last  mentioned  property  for  the  minor  key  of  C,  and  for  this 
reason  as  well  as  for  its  great  variability  (p.  345^)  it  is  largely,  perhaps  far  too 
largely  (p.  320^^),  employed  in  modem  music,  especially  for  modulations.  It  con- 
tains no  note  which  belongs  to  the  compound  tone  of  any  other  note  in  the  chord, 
but  the  three  tones  bi—d\  /may  be  regarded  as  belonging  to  the  compound  tone 
of  ^,  so  that  it  also  presents  the  appearance  of  a  chord  of  the  Ninth  in  the  form 
g+bi—d  I/— a'b-  It  therefore  imperfectly  represents  the  compomid  tone  of  the 
dominant,  with  an  intruded  tone  a*b,  and/  anda*t>  ^^J  therefore  be  regarded  as  its 
dissonant  tones.  But  the  connection  of  the  three  tones  bi-—d  |  /  with  the  compound 
tone  of  g  is  not  so  distinctly  marked  as  to  make  it  necessarjf  to  subordinate  the  pro-  «r 
gression  of  the  tones/ and  a^\}  to  that  of  bi  and  d.  At  least  the  chord  is  allowed 
to  commence  without  preparation,  and  it  is  resolved  by  the  motion  of  all  its  tones 
to  those  tones  of  the  scale  which  make  the  smallest  intervals  with  them,  for  its 
elements  are  not  sufficiently  well  connected  with  one  another  to  allow  of  wide  steps 
in  its  resolution. 

6.  The  chords  of  the  major  Seventh  in  the  direct  system  of  the  key,  as 
/-t-ai— c+Ci  and  c+«i— g^  +  ^i  in  C  major,  and  a'b+c— e^b+S'  ^^  ^  minor,  as 
already  remarked,  mainly  represent  a  major  chord  with  the  major  Seventh  as  dis- 
sonant tone.  The  major  Seventh  forms  rather  a  rough  dissonance,  and  is  decidedly 
opposed  to  the  triad  below  it,  into  which  it  will  not  fit  at  all. 

7.  The  chords  of  the  minor  Seventh  in  the  direct  system  of  the  key,  as 
ai  —  c  +  ex—g  and  ex—g  +  bi—df  give  greatest  prominence  to  the  compound  tone 
of  their  Thirds,  to  which  their  bass  seems  to  be  subjoined.  Thus  c-f  e|  —g...ai  is 
the  compound  tone  of  c  with  an  added  a,,  and  g-{-bx—d...ei  is  the  compound  tone  m 
of  g  with  an  added  Cj.  But  since  c  +  Cj— {/  and  g-\-bx—d,  being  the  principal 
triads  of  the  key,  are  constantly  recurring,  this  addition  of  a^  and  6,  respectively 
gives  by  contrast  great  prominence  to  these  tones ;  moreover,  the  a,  and  e^  in  these 
chords  of  the  Seventh  are  not  so  isolated  as  the  d'uid\  f-^-a^  —  c,  where  d  has  no  true 
Fifth  in  the  chord.  The  a,  in  aj— c+Cj-gf  has  the  Fifth  e,,  and  even  the 
Seventh  gt  which  belongs  to  its  compound  tone ;  and  in  the  same  way  the  b^  and 

*  [The  rootless  chord  of  the  Ninth  on  the  pare  its  effect  with  that  of  the  next  three 

dominant  of  C  major  is  b^—d  \  f+a^^  and  the  chords  as  given  in  the  text. — Translator,'] 
snbdominant  of   A^   minor    is    6  |  dj— /+a„  J  [The  tone  gj   of  course  represents  the 

which  would  not  be  confused  with  the  former  third  partial  of  a,.    Does  the  Author  mean 

in  just  intonation,  but  in  equal  temperament  that  the  acute  minor  Seventh  g  represents  the 

is  identical  with  it. — Translator,]  seventh  partial  '^|  for  which  it  is  49  cents,  or 

f  [This  is  the  form  in  which  the  Author  about  a  quarter  of  a  Tone  too  sharp?    The 

was  obliged  to  play  it  on  his  instrument,  which  usual  minor  Seventh  ^,  has  been  allowed  to  do 

had  /„  see  p.  317c,  note,  but  not  Y.    On  the  so,  although  27  cents  too  sharp.    Perhaps  the 

Harmonical  play  c  +  c,— y...'6l)...o  and  com-  expression  *  even  the  Seventh'  (alien/alls  auch 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


35©  PKOGEESSION  OP  PAETS.  pabt  hi. 

dotei—g+bi—d  may  be  considered  to  belong  to  the  compound  tone  of  e,.  Hence 
the  tone  a^  in  the  first  and  Ci  in  the  second  are  not  necessarily  subject  to  the  laws 
of  the  resolution  of  dissonant  notes. 

Writers  on  harmony  are  accustomed  to  consider  the  normal  position  of  all  these 
chords  to  be  that  of  the  chord  of  the  Seventh,  and  to  call  the  lowest  tone  its  root. 
Perhaps  it  would  be  more  natural  to  consider  c  +  e,  —g...ai  as  the  principal  position 
of  the  chord  ai— c+^i— gr  and  c  as  its  root.  But  such  a  chord  is  a  compound 
tone  of  c  with  an  inclination  to  aj ,  and  in  modulations  this  intrusion  of  the  tone 
of  ai  is  utilised  for  proceeding  to  those  chords  related  to  aj  which  are  not  related 
to  the  chord  c+e,  —g,  for  example  to  d^  — /+ai.  In  the  same  way  we  can  proceed 
from  ^  +  6,— d...ei  to  ai— c+«i,  which  would  be  a  jump  from  ^-f  6,— (^.  For 
modulation,  therefore,  the  a^  and  ei  are  essential  parts  of  these  chords  respectively, 
and  in  this  practical  light  they  might  be  called  the  fundamental  tones  of  their 
I  respective  chords. 

8.  The  chord  of  the  Seventh  on  the  tonic  of  the  minor  hey,  c-e^b+f^-f  ^i,  is 
seldom  used,  because  b^  in  the  minor  key  belongs  essentially  to  ascending  motion, 
and  a  resolved  Seventh  habitually  descends.  Hence  it  would  be  always  better  to 
form  the  chord  c— e*t)+^  — 6^l>,  which  is  similar  to  the  chords  considered  in  No.  7. 


CHAPTER  XVm. 

LAWS  OF  THE  PBOGRESSION  OF  PARTS. 


Up  to  this  point  we  have  considered  only  the  relations  of  the  tones  in  a  piece  of 
«-  music  with  its  tonic,  and  of  its  chords  with  its  tonic  chord.  On  these  relations 
depends  the  connection  of  the  parts  of  a  mass  of  tone  into  one  coherent  whole. 
But  besides  this  the  succession  of  the  tones  and  chords  must  be  regulated  by  natural 
relations.  The  mass  of  sound  thus  becomes  more  intimately  bound  up  together, 
and,  as  a  general  rule,  we  must  aim  at  producing  such  a  connection,  although, 
exceptionally,  peculiar  expression  may  necessitate  the  selection  of  a  more  violent 
and  less  obvious  plan  of  progression.  In  the  development  of  the  scale  we  saw  that 
the  connection  of  all  the  notes  by  means  of  their  relation  to  the  tonic,  if  originally 
perceived  at  all,  was  at  most  but  very  dimly  seen,  and  was  apparently  replaced  by 
the  chain  of  Fifths ;  at  any  rate,  the  latter  alone  was  sufficiently  developed  to  be 
recognised  in  the  Pythagorean  construction  of  tonal  systems.  But  by  the  side  of 
our  strongly  developed  feeling  for  the  tonic  in  modem  harmonic  music,  the  necessity 
for  a  linked  connection  of  individual  tones  and  chords  is  still  recognised,  although 
the  chain  of  Fifths,  which  originally  connected  the  tones  of  the  scale,  as 

If  f±c±g±     d     ±a±e±b, 

has  been  interrupted  by  the  introduction  of  perfect  major  Thirds,  and  now  appears 
as 

/±  o±^g  ±d...di±^i±:^i±,  *i- 

The  musical  connection  between  two  consecutive  notes  may  be  effected : 

I.  By  the  relation  of  their  compound  tones. 

This  is  either : 

a.)  direct,  when  the  two  consecutive  tones  form  a  perfectly  consonant  interval, 
in  which  case,  as  we  have  previously  seen,  one  of  the  clearly  perceptible  partial 
tones  of  the  first  note  is  identical  with  one  of  the  second.     The  pitch  of  the  follow- 

die  Septime)  is  intended  to  shew  that  this       —  gf,  and  the  chord  of  a  is  ||a-4-cS  —  e-^.  Bat 
view  is  rather  too  loose.    In  eqaal  tempera-      this  is  mere  confosion. — TranslaiorJ] 
ment,  indeed,  the  dissonant  chord  is  g  a-c  +  6 


Digitized  by 


^.joogle 


CHAP.  XVIII.  MUSICAL  CONNECTION  OP  TONES.  351 

ing  compound  tone  is  then  clearly  determined  for  the  ear.  This  is  the  best  and 
surest  kind  of  connection.  The  closest  relationship  of  this  kind  exists  when  the 
voice  jumps  a  whole  Octave ;  but  this  is  not  usual  in  melodies,  except  with  the 
bass,  as  the  alteration  of  pitch  is  felt  to  be  too  sudden  for  the  upper  part.  Next 
to  this  comes  the  jump  of  a  Fifth  or  Fourth,  both  of  which  are  very  definite 
and  clear.  After  these  follow  the  steps  of  a  major  Sixth  or  major  Third,  both  of 
which  can  be  readily  taken,  but  some  uncertainty  begins  to  arise  in  the  case  of 
minor  Sixths  and  minor  Thirds.  Esthetically  it  should  be  remarked,  that  of  all 
the  melodic  steps  just  mentioned,  the  major  Sixth  and  major  Third  have,  I  might 
almost  say,  the  highest  degree  of  thorough  beauty.  This  possibly  depends  upon 
their  position  at  the  limit  of  clearly  intelligible  intervals.  The  steps  of  a  Fifth  or 
Fourth  are  too  clear,  and  hence  are,  as  it  were,  drily  intelligible;  the  steps  of 
minor  Thirds,  and  especially  minor  Sixths,  begin  to  sound  indeterminate.  The 
major  Thirds  and  major  Sixths  seem  to  hold  the  right  balance  between  darkness  ^ 
and  light.  The  major  Sixth  and  major  Third  seem  also  to  stand  in  the  same 
relation  to  the  other  intervals  harmonically. 

b.)  indirect,  of  the  second  degree  only.    This  occurs  in  the  regular  progression 
of  the  scale,  proceeding  by  Tones  or  Semitones.     For  example  : 

c  ...  d  d  ...  $1  Bi      f 

^IT         ^"5^        ^c^ 

The  whole  major  Tone  c ...  d  proceeds  from  the  Fourth  to  the  Fifth  of  the 
auxiliary  tone  G,  which  Eameau  supposed  to  be  subjoined  as  the  fundamental  bass 
of  the  above  melodic  progression.  The  minor  Tone  d...e\  proceeds  from  the  Fifth 
to  the  major  Sixth  of  the  auxiHary  tone  (7,  and  the  Semitone  61 .../  from  the  major 
Third  to  the  Fourth  of  the  auxiliary  tone  C  But  in  order  that  these  auxiliary 
tones  may  readily  occur  to  both  singer  and  hearer,  they  must  be  among  the  f 
principal  tones  of  the  key.  Thus  the  step  a^.^.h^  in  the  major  scale  of  G  causes 
the  singers  a  little  trouble,  although  it  is  only  the  interval  of  a  major  Tone,  and 
could  be  easily  referred  to  the  auxiliary  tone  E^,  But  the  sound  of  ei  is  not  so  firm 
and  ready  in  the  mind,  as  the  sound  of  G  and  its  Fifth  G  and  Fourth  F.  Hence  the 
Hexachord  of  Guide  of  Arezzo,  which  was  the  normal  scale  for  singers  throughout 
the  middle  ages,  ended  at  the  Sixth.*  This  Hexachord  was  sung  with  different 
pitches  of  the  first  note,  but  always  formed  the  same  melody : 


m 

Be 

Mi 

Fa 

Sol 

La 

either 

G 

A 

B 

C 

D 

E 

or 

C 

D 

E 

F 

G 

A 

or 

F 

G 

A 

B\} 

C 

D 

So  that  the  interval  Mi.., Fa  always  marked  the  Semitone. t 

For  the  same  reason  Eameau  preferred,  in  the  minor  scale,  to  refer  tlie  steps  ^ 
d...e't)  aiid  e^[}...f  to  G  and  G  as  auxiliary  tones,  rather  than  to  B}},  the  Seventh  of 

*  For  the  same  reason  d'Alembert  explains  certainly  ased  for  training  singers  in  meantone 

the  limits  of  the  old  Greek  heptachord,  by  temperament.    It  could  not  have  been  used  for 

means  of  two  connected  tetraohords —  just  intonation,  because  the  melody  cdef  is 

K      Q      g     ^      f     Q     a  assumed  to  be  identical with^  a  6  cin the  same 

''  "*  *' '"  scale,  whereas  in  just  intonation  c  204  d  182  «» 

in  which  the  step  a ...  &  is  avoided.    But  this  112/ and ^  i82a|  20461 112c' are  different.   For 

explanation  would  only  suit  a  key  in  which  c  an  excellent  account  of  the  Hexachord  see 

was  the  tonic,  and  this  ¥ras  probably  not  the  Mr.  Bockstro's  article  *  Hexachord,'  in  Grove's 

case  for  the  ancient  Greek  scale.  Dictionary.    To  shew,  however,  how  intona- 

t  [Prof.  Helmholtz  leaves  the  intonation  tions  are  mixed  up,  it  may  be  observed  that  he 

unmarked.    Guido  d' Arezzo,  the  presumed  in-  illustrates  the  use  of  the  Hexachord  in  *  Beal 

ventor  of  the  Hexachord,  is  said  to  have  intro-  Fugue '  by  an  example  of  Palestrina,  who  lived 

duced  it  about  1024  a.]>.,  that  is  long  before  in  the  sixteenth  century,  and  is  often  credited 

meantone  temperament  existed.     Hence   we  with  just  intonation,  but  who  being  junior  to 

must  assume  Pythagorean  intonation  (see  p.  Salinas  and  Zarlino  must  have  used  meantone 

3i3fi).    Yet  in  later  times  the  Hexachord  was  temperament.— Tratwiator.] 


Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


352  MUSICAL  CONNECTION  OF  TONES.  part  in. 

the  descending  scale,*  which  had  not  a  sufficiently  close  relationship  to  the  tonic, 
and  hence  was  not  well  enough  impressed  on  the  singer's  mind  for  such  a  purpose. 
Taking  g  and  c,  the  Octaves  of  G  and  C  as  the  auxihary  tones,  the  motion  in 
d...e^\}  is  from  the  Fourth  below  g  to  the  major  Third  below  it,  and  in  e^\}...f  from 
the  major  Sixth  below  c  to  the  Fifth  below  it.  On  the  other  hand,  it  is  impossible 
to  reduce  the  step  a^\}...bx  [cents  274]  in  the  minor  scale  to  any  relationship  of  the 
second  degree.  [See  p.  301c,  note  *.]  It  is  therefore  also  decidedly  unmelodic 
and  had  to  be  entirely  avoided  in  the  old  homophonic  music,  just  as  the  steps 
of  the  false  Fifths  and  Fourths,  as  6 1 . . ./  [cents  6 1 o] ,  or  / . . . 5 /  [cents  590] .  Hence 
the  alterations  in  the  ascending  and  descending  miaor  scales  already  mentioned. 

In  modem  harmonic  music  many  of  these  difficulties  have  disappeared,  or 
become  less  sensible,  because  correct  harmonisation  can  exhibit  the  connections 
which  are  absent  in  the  melodic  progression  of  an  unaccompanied  voice.     Hence 

%  also  it  is  much  easier  to  take  a  part  at  sight  in  a  harmony,  written  in  pianoforte 
score,  which  shews  its  relations,  than  to  sing  it  from  an  unconnected  part.  The 
former  shews  how  the  tone  to  be  sung  is  connected  with  the  whole  harmony,  the 
latter  gives  only  its  connection  with  the  adjacent  tones.f 

2.  Tones  may  be  connected  by  their  ajp'proxhnation  in  pitch. 
This  relation  has  been  considered  previously  with  reference  to  the  leading  note. 
The  same  holds  good  for  the  intercalated  tones  in  chromatic  passages.  For 
example,  if  in  C  major,  we  replace  C...D  hj  C.CJff  ..D,  this  Cj  has  no  relation 
either  of  the  first  or  second  degree  with  the  tonic  (7,  and  also  no  harmonic  or 
modulational  significance.  It  is  nothing  but  a  step  intercalated  between  two 
tones,  which  has  no  relation  to  the  scale,  and  only  serves  to  render  its  discontinuous 
progression  more  Uke  the  gliding  motion  of  natural  speech,  or  weeping  or  howling. 
The  Greeks  carried  this  subdivision  still  further  than  we  do  at  present,  by  splitting 
up  a  Semitone  into  two  parts  in  their  enharmonic  system  (p.  265a).   Notwithstand- 

^  ing  the  strangeness  of  the  tone  to  be  struck,  chromatic  progression  in  Semitones 
can  be  executed  with  sufficient  certainty  to  allow  it  to  be  used  in  modulational 
transitions  for  the  purpose  of  suddenly  reaching  very  distant  keys. 

ItaHan  melodies  are  especially  rich  in  such  intercalated  tones.  Investigations 
of  the  laws^  under  which  they  occur  will  be  found  in  two  essays  of  Sig.  A.  Basevi.J 
The  rule  is  without  exception  that  tones  foreign  to  the  scale  can  be  introduced  only 
when  they  differ  by  a  Semitone  §  from  the  note  of  the  scale  on  to  which  they 
resolve,  while  any  tones  belonging  to  the  scale  itself  can  be  freely  introduced 
although  out  of  harmony  with  the  accompaniment,  and  even  requiring  the  step  of 
a  whole  Tone  for  their  resolution. 

*  [The  Anthor  writes  £b,  and  calls  it '  the  tones,  the  small  90  and  the  large  1 14  cents,  and 
Seventh  of  the  descending  scale '  of  C  minor,  the  rule  was  to  make  the  Semitone  closest  to  the 
which,  however,  is  B'b,  and  this  answers  for  note  to  which  it  led,  thus  cii4c](9od,  (i  114 
the  first  interval  (2  ...  e^ b,  owing  to  6^ b  +  d^  and  db  9od,  And  this  notation  was  retained  even 
^  e^b  ±  6*b  ;  but  it  does  not  answer  to  the  second  in  meantone  temperament,  where  the  relations 
interval  e'b  .../  as  /  ...6*b  is  dissonant,  and  it  were  reversed,  as  c  76  cj  117  d^  d  76  db  117  c; 
would  not  do  to  use  &b,  although  6b  ±/  is  con-  but  practically  this  made  no  difference  except 
sonant,  because  e'b  ...  &b  is  dissonant.  Buta'b  .  to  the  singer,  as  the  player  had  only  one  Semi- 
would  do,  as  we  see  from  a'bie^b,  /— a'b.  tone  at  command.  Ttus  writing  is  still  con- 
Hence  if  the  text  gives  Rameau's  notes,  he  must  tinned  in  equal  temperament,  although  the 
have  been  misled  by  temperament. — Trans-  two  Semitones  are  now  equalised  as  100  cents, 
lator.]  thus  c  100  cH  100  d,  and  d  100  db  100  c.    Bat 

f  [Hence  any  means  of  shewing  the  relation  in  just  intonation  We  have  Semitones  of  varioos 

of  each  tone  to  the  tonic  of  the  moment,  as  in  dimensions,  c  114  eft  90  c2,  c  112  d^b  92  d,  c  92 

the  Tonic  Solfa  system,  materially  facilitates  c,](  112  d,  c  90  db  114  d,  c  70  CjS  134  d ;  which 

sight-singing,  as  perhaps  the  use  of  the  duode-  of  these  is  the  player  to  play,  or  the  singer  to 

nal  (App.  XX.  sect.  E.  art.  26)  when  thoroughly  sing  (a  question  of  importance  when  each  part 

understood  might  also  do. — Translator.']  is  sustained  by  many  unaccompanied  voices)  ? 

X  Introduction   d    un   nouveau    System  Practically  the  player  will  take  the  most  handy 

d*Harmoniet  traduit  par  L.  DeUtre ;  Florence,  interval,  and  the  singers  must  arrange  in  re- 

1855.    Studj  sulV  Armoniat  Firenze,  1865.  hearsal,  but  would  possibly  take  c  92  c^t  1 12  d, 

§  [Of  course  those  who  laid  down  the  rule  d  92  d'b  112  c,  as  these  are  the  intervals  used 

thought  only  of  a  tempered  Semitone.    But  in  in  modulation  from  /  to  /,S  to  the  dominant, 

Pythagorean  temperament  there  were  two  Semi-  and  6,  to  6  b  to  the  Bubdominant. — Translator.] 


Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


CHAP.  xvm.  EESOLUTION  OF  DISSONANCE.  353 

In  the  same  way  steps  of  a  whole  Tone  may  be  made,  provided  the  notes  lie  in 
the  scale,  when  they  serve  merely  to  connect  two  other  tones  which  belong  to 
chords.  These  are  the  so-called  passing  or  changing  notes.  Thus  if  while  the 
triad  of  C  major  is  sustained,  a  voice  sings  the  passage  c...d...ei.,f...g,  the  two 
notes  d  and  /  do  not  suit  the  chord,  and  have  no  relation  to  the  harmony,  but 
are  simply  justified  by  the  melodic  progression  of  the  single  voice.  It  is  usual  to 
place  these  passing  notes  on  the  unaccented  parts  of  the  bar,  and  to  give  them  a 
short  duration.  Thus  in  the  above  example  c,  e,,  ^  would  fall  on  accented  parts 
of  the  bar.  Then  d  is  the  passing  note  between  c  and  ei  and /between  e^  and  g. 
It  is  essential  for  their  intelligibihty  that  they  should  make  steps  of  Semitones  or 
whole  Tones.  They  thus  produce  a  simple  melodic  progression,  which  flows  on 
freely,  without  giving  any  prominence  to  the  dissonances  produced. 

Even  in  Che  essentially  dissonant  chords  the  rule  is,  that  dissonant  tones  which  in- 
trude isolatedly  on  the  mass  of  the  other  tones  must  proceed  in  a  melodic  progression,  f 
which  can  be  easily  understood  and  easily  performed.  And  since  the  feeling  for 
the  natural  relations  of  such  an  isolated  tone  is  almost  overpowered  by  the  simul- 
taneous sound  of  the  other  tones  which  force  themselves  much  more  strongly  on 
the  attention,  both  singer  and  hearer  are  thrown  upon  the  gradual  diatonic  pro- 
gression as  the  only  means  of  clearly  fixing  the  melodic  relations  of  a  dissonant 
note  of  this  description.  Hence  it  is  generally  necessary  that  a  dissonant  note 
should  enter  and  leave  the  chord  by  degrees  of  the  scale. 

Chords  must  be  considered  essentially  dissonant,  in  which  the  dissonant  notes 
do  not  enter  as  passing  notes  over  a  sustained  chord,  but  are  either  accompanied 
by  an  especial  chord,  differing  firom  the  preceding  and  following  chords,  or  else  are 
rendered  so  prominent  by  their  duration  or  accentuation,  that  they  cannot  possibly 
escape  the  attention  of  the  hearer.  It  has  been  already  remarked  that  these  chords 
are  not  used  for  their  own  sakes,  but  principally  as  a  means  of  increasing  the 
feeling  of  onward  progression  in  the  composition.  Hence  it  follows  for  the  motion  IT 
of  the  dissonant  note,  that  when  it  enters  and  leaves  the  chord,  it  will  either  ascend 
on  each  occasion  or  descend  on  each.  If  we  allowed  it  to  reverse  its  motion  in  the 
second  half,  and  thus  return  to  its  original  position,  there  would  seem  to  have  been 
no  motive  for  the  dissonance.  It  would  in  that  case  have  been  better  to  leave  the 
note  at  rest  in  its  consonant  position.  A  motion  which  returns  to  its  origin  and 
creates  a  dissonance  by  the  way,  had  better  be  avoided ;  it  has  no  object. 

Secondly  it  may  be  laid  down  as  a  rule,  that  the  motion  of  the  dissonant  note 
should  not  be  such  as  to  make  the  chord  consonant  without  any  change  in  the 
other  notes.  For  a  dissonance  which  disappears  of  itself  provided  we  wait  for  the 
next  step,  gives  no  impetus  to  the  progress  of  the  harmony.  It  sounds  poor  and 
unjustified.  This  is  the  principal  reason  why  chords  of  the  Seventh  which  have 
to  be  resolved  by  the  motion  of  the  Seventh,  can  only  permit  the  Seventh  to 
descend.  For  if  the  Seventh  ascended  in  the  scale,  it  would  pass  into  the  octave 
of  the  lowest  tone,  and  the  dissonance  of  the  chord  would  disappear.  When  Bach,  % 
Mozart,  and  others  use  such  progressions  for  chords  of  the  dominant  Seventh,  the 
Seventh  has  the  effect  of  a  passing  note,  and  must  be  so  treated.  In  that  case  it 
has  no  effect  on  the  progression  of  the  harmony. 

The  pitch  of  a  single  dissonant  note  in  a  chord  of  many  parts  is  determined 
with  greatest  certainty,  when  it  has  been  previously  heard  as  a  consonance  in  the 
preceding  chord,  and  is  merely  sustained  while  the  new  chord  is  introduced.  Thus 
if  we  take  the  following  succession  of  chords  : 

0...d...  g  +  b^ 
c  +«!—  g  +  bi 

the  hi  in  the  first  chord  is  determined  by  its  consonance  with  G.  It  simply  remains 
while  the  tones  c  and  Bi  are  introduced  in  place  of  G  and  d,  and  thus  becomes  a 
dissonance  in  the  chord  of  the  Seventh  c-^ei—  g+b^.  In  this  case  the  dissonance 
is  said  to  be  prepared.    This  was  the  only  way  in  which  dissonances  might  be 

Digitized  by^^^QlC 


354  RESOLUTION  OF  DISSONANCE.  pabt  ra. 

introduced  down  to  the  end  of  the  sixteenth  century.  Prepared  dissonances  produce 
a  peculiarly  powerful  effect :  a  part  of  the  preceding  chord  lingers  on,  and  has  to 
be  forced  from  its  position  by  the  following  chord.  In  this  way,  an  effort  to  advance 
against  opposing  obstacles  wliich  only  slowly  yield,  is  very  effectively  expressed. 
And  for  the  same  reason  the  newly  introduced  chord  (c  4  Ci  —  5^  in  the  last 
example)  must  enter  on  a  strongly  accented  part  of  the  bar ;  as  it  would  otheonrise 
not  sufficiently  express  exertion.  The  resolution  of  the  prepared  dissonance,  on 
the  contrary,  naturally  falls  on  an  unaccented  part  of  the  bar.  Nothing  sonnds 
worse  than  dissonances  played  or  sung  in  a  dragging  or  uncertain  manner.  In 
that  case  they  appear  to  be  simply  out  of  tune.  They  are,  as  a  rule,  only  justified 
by  expressing  energy  and  vigorous  progress. 

Such  prepared  dissonances,  termed  sttspensions,  may  occur  in  many  other  chords 
besides  those  of  the  Seventh.     For  example  : 

''  Preparation :     G  .,.c   +  «i 

Suspension  :     G  ...c   ...d 
Resolution    :     G  -\-  Bi—  d 

The  tone  c  is  the  prepared  dissonance ;  in  the  second  chord,  which  must  faXL  on 
an  accented  part  of  the  bar,  d  the  Fifth  of  G  is  introduced  and  generates  the  dis- 
sonance c..,d,  and  then  c  must  give  way,  and  according  to  our  second  rule,  must 
go  further  from  d,  which  results  in  the  resolution  G  -{-  Bi  ^  d.  The  chords  might 
also  be  played  in  the  inverse  order,  and  then  d  would  be  a  prepared  dissonance  which 
was  forced  away  by  c.  But  this  is  not  so  good,  because  descending  motion  is  better 
suited  than  ascending  motion  to  an  extruded  note.  Heightened  pitch  always  gives 
us  involuntarily  the  impression  of  greater  effort,  because  we  have  continually  to 
exert  our  voice  in  order  to  reach  high  tones.  The  dissonant  note  on  descending 
seems  to  yield  suitably  to  superior  force,  but  on  ascending  it  as  it  were  rises  by  its 

^  own  exertion.  But  circumstances  may  render  the  latter  course  suitable,  and  its 
occurrence  is  not  unfrequent. 

In  the  other  case,  especially  frequent  for  chords  of  the  Seventh,  when  the  dis- 
sonant note  is  not  prepared  but  is  struck  simultaneously  with  the  chord  to  which  it 
is  dissonant,  the  significance  of  the  dissonance  is  different.  Since  these  unprepared 
Sevenths  must  usually  enter  by  the  descent  of  the  preceding  note,  they  may  be 
always  considered  as  descending  from  the  Octave  of  the  root  of  their  chord,  by 
supposing  a  consonant  major  or  minor  chord  having  the  same  root  as  the  chord 
of  the  Seventh  to  be  inserted  between  that  and  the  preceding  chord.  In  this  case 
the  entrance  of  the  Seventh  merely  indicates  that  this  consonant  chord  begins  to 
break  up  immediately  and  that  the  melodic  progression  gives  a  new  direction  to  the 
harmony.  This  new  direction,  leading  to  the  chord  of  resolution,  must  be  empha- 
sised, and  hence  the  dissonance  necessarily  falls  on  the  preceding  unaccented  pari 
of  a  bar. 

^  The  introduction  of  an  isolated  dissonant  note  into  a  chord  of  several  parts 
cannot  generally  be  used  as  the  expression  of  exertion,  but  this  character  will 
attach  to  the  introduction  of  a  chord  as  against  a  single  note,  supposing  that  this 
single  note  is  not  too  powerful.  Hence  it  lies  in  the  nature  of  things  that  the 
first  kind  of  introduction  takes  place  on  unaccented  and  the  last  on  accented  parts 
of  a  bar. 

These  rules  for  the  introduction  of  dissonances  may  be  often  neglected  for  the 
chords  of  the  Seventh  in  the  reverted  system,  in  which  the  Fourth  and  Second  of 
the  scale  oecur,  and  notes  from  the  subdominant  side  are  mixed  up  with  notes  from 
the  dominant  side.  These  chords  may  also  be  introduced  to  enhance  the  dynamical 
impression  of  the  advancing  harmony,  for  they  have  the  effect  of  keeping  the 
extent  of  the  key  perpetually  before  the  feeling  of  the  hearer,  and  this  object 
justifies  their  existence. 

Of  several  voices  which  are  leaving  the  chord  of  the  tonic  C,  it  is  quite  easy 
for  some  to  pass  on  to  notes  of  the  dominant  chord  g  +  bi  —  d,  and  for  others  to 

Digitized  by  V^OOQIC 


CHAP.  xvin.  CHORDAL  SEQUENCES.  355 

proceed  to  the  notes  of  the  subdominant  chords/  +  «i  —  c  or/  —  a't>  +  c,  as  each 
voice  will  be  able  to  strike  the  new  note  with  perfect  certainty,  on  account  of  the 
close  relationship  between  the  chords.  When,  however,  the  dissonant  chord  has  been 
thus  formed  and  sounded,  the  dissonant  notes  will  have  the  feeling  for  their  more 
distant  relations  obscured  by  the  strangeness  of  the  other  parts  of  the  chord,  and 
must  generally  proceed  according  to  the  rule  of  resolution  of  dissonances.  Thus 
the  singer  who  sounds  /  in  the  chord  g-^-bi^d  \f,  would  vainly  endeavour  to 
picture  to  himself  the  sound  of  the  ai  which  is  related  to  /  with  sufficient  clearness 
to  leap  up  or  down  to  it  with  certainty ;  but  he  is  easily  able  to  execute  the  small 
step  of  half  a  Tone,  by  which  /  descends  to  6]  in  the  chord  ci-ei—g.  But  the 
note  g  itself,  on  the  other  hand,  having  its  own  compound  tone  approximatively 
indicated  by  the  chord  of  the  Seventh,  has  no  difficulty  in  passing  by  a  leap  to  its 
related  notes,  as  c  for  example,  or  bi  to  g. 

In  the  chords  hi  —d  \  f-\-ai  and  61  —d  |  /— a^t),  in  which  neither  dominant  nor  ^ 
Bubdomiiiant  prevails,  it  would  not  be  advisable  to  let  any  note  proceed  by  a  leap. 

And  it  would  also  not  be  advisable  to  pass  by  a  leap  into  the  chords  of  the 
reverted  system  from  any  other  chord  but  the  tonic,  because  that  chord  alone  is 
related  to  both  dominant  and  subdominant  chords  at  the  same  time. 

It  is  not  possible  to  pass  to  chords  of  the  Seventh  in  the  direct  system,  from 
another  chord  related  to  both  extremities  of  the  chord  of  the  Seventh,  and  hence 
in  this  case  the  dissonance  must  be  introduced  in  accordance  with  the  strict  rules. 

Musicians  are  divided  in  opinion  as  to  the  proper  treatment  of  the  subdominant 
chord  with  an  added  Sixth,  f-\-ai  —  c...d  in  C  major.  The  rule  of  Eameau  is 
probably  correct  (p.  347^),  making  d  the  dissonant  note,  to  be  resolved  by  rising 
to  Bi.  This  is  also  decidedly  the  most  harmonious  kind  of  resolution.  Modem 
theorists,  on  the  other  hand,  regard  this  chord  as  a  chord  of  the  Seventh  on  d^  and 
take  c  as  the  dissonant  note  to  be  resolved  by  descent ;  whereas  when  c  remains,  d 
is  quite  free  and  may  therefore  even  descend.  cr 

Chordal  Sequences. 

Just  as  the  older  homophonic  music  required  the  notes  of  a  melody  to  be  linked 
together,  modern  music  endeavours  to  link  together  the  series  of  chords  occurring 
in  a  tissue  of  harmony,  and  it  thus  obtains  much  greater  freedom  in  the  melodic 
succession  of  individual  notes,  because  the  natural  relationship  of  the  notes  is' 
much  more  decisively  and  emphatically  marked  in  harmonic  music  than  in  homo- 
phonic  melody.  This  desire  for  linking  the  chords  together  was  but  sHghtly 
developed  in  the  sixteenth  century.  The  great  Italian  masters  of  this  period  allow 
the  chords  of  the  key  to  succeed  each  other  in  leaps  which  are  often  surprising, 
and  which  we  should  at  present  admit  only  in  exceptional  cases.  But  during  the 
seventeenth  century  the  feeling  for  this  peculiarity  of  harmony  also  was  developed, 
so  that  we  find  Eameau  laying  down  distinct  rules  on  the  subject  in  the  beginning 
of  the  eighteenth  century.  In  reference  to  his  conception  of  fundamental  bass,  f 
Bameau  worded  his  rule  thus :  '  Ths  fundamental  bass  ma/y,  as  a  general  rule, 
proceed  only  in  perfect  Fifths  or  Thirds,  upwards  or  downwards.'  According  to 
our  view  the  fundamental  bass  of  a  chord  is  that  compound  tone  which  is  either 
exclusively  or  principally  represented  by  the  notes  of  the  chord.  In  this  sense 
Bameau's  rule  coincides  with  that  for  the  melodic  progression  of  a  single  note  to 
its  nearest  related  notes.  The  compound  tone  of  a  chord,  like  the  voice  of  a 
melody,  may  only  proceed  to  its  nearest  related  notes.  It  is  much  more  difficult 
to  assign  a  meaning  to  progression  by  relationship  in  the  second  degree  for  chords 
than  for  separate  notes,  and  similarly  for  progression  in  small  diatonic  degrees 
-without  relationship.  Hence  Rameau's  rule  for  the  progression  of  the  funda- 
mental bass  is  on  the  whole  stricter  than  the  rules  for  the  melodic  progression 
of  a  single  voice. 

Thus  if  we  take  the  chord  c+^i  — gr,  which  belongs  to  the  compound  tone  of  C, 
we  may  pass  by  Fifths  to  ^+61— d,  the  compound  tone  of  G,  or  to/-f  aj  — c  the 

Digitized  by  V^jOAQIC 


356  CHORDAL  SEQUENCES.  pabt  in. 

compound  tone  of  F.  Both  of  these  chords  are  directly  related  to  the  first 
0+^1— 9»  because  each  has  one  note  in  common  with  it,  g  and  c  respectively. 

But  we  can  also  allow  the  compound  tone  to  proceed  in  Thirds,  and  then  we 
obtain  minor  chords,  that  is,  provided  we  keep  to  the  same  scale.  The  transition 
from  the  compound  tone  of  C  to  that  of  ^i  is  expressed  by  the  sequence  of  chords 
c+e^—g  and  e,— ^+&if  which  are  related  by  having  two  notes,  «i,  gr,  in  common. 
,  The  sequence  c+^i— ^  and  a^— c+^i  from  the  compound  tone  of  C  to  that  of  Ai 
is  of  the  same  kind.  The  latter  is  even  more  natural  than  the  former,  because  the 
chord  ai  —  c+^i  represents  imperfectly  the  compound  tone  of  Ax  into  which  that  of 
C  intrudes,  so  that  the  compound  tone  of  0,  which  was  clearly  given  in  the  pre- 
ceding chord,  persists  with  two  of  its  tones,  c,  ^i,  in  the  second  chord,  a  relation 
which  did  not  exist  in  the  former  case. 

But  if  we  prefer  to  leave  the  key  of  G  major,  we  can  pass  to  perfect  compound 
f  tones  in  Thirds,  as  from  c  +  «i  —  ^  to  fii  +  g^  —  hi  or  a^  +  c^  —  6i,  as  is  very 
usual  in  modulations. 

Bameau  will  not  allow  a  simple  diatonic  progression  of  the  fundamental  baas  of 
consonant  triads,  except  where  major  and  minor  chords  alternate,  as  from  g-¥hi^d 
to  a|— c+<?it  that  is  from  the  compound  tone  of  O  to  that  oi  Ai,  but  cedls  this  a 
*  hcence.'  In  reality  this  progression  is  readily  explicable  from  our  point  of  view, 
by  considering  aj—  c  +  ^i  as  a  compound  tone  of  G  with  an  intrusive  ai.  The 
transition  is  then  one  of  the  usual  close  relationship,  from  the  compound  tone  of 
G  to  that  of  C,  and  the  aj  appears  as  a  mere  appendage  to  the  latter.  Every 
minor  chord  represents  two  compound  tones  in  an  imperfect  manner.  Bameaa 
first  formulated  this  ambiguity  {double  emploi)  for  the  minor  chord  with  added 
Seventh,  which,  in  the  form  ^i  — /  +  ai—  c,  may  represent  the  compound  tone  of 
Di,  and  in  the  form/+ai—  c.d  that  of  Fy  or  in  Bameau's  language  its  funda- 
mental bass  might  be  Di  or  F,*  In  this  chord  of  the  Seventh  the  ambiguity  is 
f  more  marked,  because  it  contains  the  compound  tone  of  F  more  completely ;  but 
the  ambiguity  belongs  in  a  less  marked  degree  to  the  simple  chord  also. 

With  the  false  cadence  in  the  major  key 

g  +  6i— d        to        ai  — c  +  Ci 

must  be  associated  the  corresponding  cadence  in  the  minor  key» 

g  +  bi—d       to        a*t>— c  +  c^t> 

where  the  chord  a* |> — o + e^ t>  replaces  the  normal  resolution  c^e^\}+g.  But  here 
there  is  only  a  single  note  of  the  compound  tone  of  G  remaining,  and  the  false 
cd.dence  therefore  becomes  much  more  striking.  It  will  be  rendered  milder  by 
adding  the  Seventh /to  the  G  chord,  because/ is  related  to  a^\}. 

When  two  chords  having  only  a  relationship  of  the  second  degree,  are  placed 
in  juxtaposition,  we  usually  feel  the  transition  to  be  very  abrupt.  But  if  the  chord 
^  which  connects  them  is  one  of  the  principal  chords  of  the  key,  and  has  con- 
sequently been  frequently  heard,  the  effect  is  not  so  striking.  Thus  in  the  final 
cadence  it  is  not  unusual  to  see  the  succession/ -h  ai—  c  and  y+6i  —  d,  the  two 
chords  being  related  through  the  tonic  chord  c+ei^  g,  thus  : 


/  +  ai-c  5r  +  6i-d 

c  —  «i  —  ^ 
> , 

Generally  we  must  remember  that  all  these  rules  of  progression  are  subject 
to  many  exceptions,  partly  because  expression  may  require  exceptional  abruptness 
of  transition,  and  partly  because  the  hearer's  recollection  of  previous  chords  may 

*  [Of  ooorse  Bamean,  writing  in  tempered      and  f+a^-^c.^d  were  to  him  identioaL    See 
notation,  did  not  distinguiBh  d,  and  <2,  so  that      pp.  340a,  345a,  34&1. — Translator.] 
the  actual  notes  in  the  two  chords  c{,  — /+ a,  •  e 


Digitized  by 


Google 


CHAP.  xvin.  CHORDAL  SEQUENCES.  357 

saffioiently  strengthen  a  naturally  weak  relationship.  It  is  clearly  an  entirely  false 
position  which  teachers  of  harmony  have  assumed,  in  declaring  this  or  that  to  be 
'  forbidden '  in  music.  In  point  of  fact  nothing  musical  is  absolutely  forbidden, 
and  all  rules  for  the  progression  of  parts  are  actually  violated  in  the  most  effective 
pieces  of  the  greatest  composers.  It  would  have  been  much  better  to  proceed  from 
the  principle  that  certain  transitions,  which  are  disallowed,  produce  striking  and 
unusual  effects  upon  the  hearer,  and  consequently  are  unsuitable  except  for  the 
expression  of  what  is  unusual.  Generally  speaking,  the  object  of  the  rules  laid 
down  by  theorists  is  to  keep  up  a  well-connected  flow  of  melody  and  harmony,  and 
make  its  course  readily  intelligible.  If  that  is  what  we  aim  at,  we  had  best  observe 
their  restrictions.  But  it  caimot  be  denied  that  a  too  anxious  avoidance  of  what  is 
unusual  places  us  in  danger  of  becoming  trivial  and  dull,  while,  on  the  other  hand, 
inconsiderate  and  frequent  infringement  of  rules  makes  compositions  eccentric  and 
unconnected.  % 

When  disconnected  triads  would  come  together  it  is  frequently  advantageous  to 
transform  them  into  chords  of  the  Seventh,  and  thus  create  a  bond  between  them. 
In  place  of  the  preceding  sequence  of  two  triads 

/-hai— c     to      gf  +  5i— d 

we  can  use  a  sequence  of  chords  of  the  Seventh  which  r^resent  the  same  00m- 
pound  tones 

/  +  tti—  c  ...d     to      g  +  &i—  d  I  /. 

In  this  case  two  of  the  four  notes  remain  unchanged ;  in  the  chord  of  F,  the 
d  belongs  to  the  compound  tone  of  the  dominant,  and  in  the  chord  of  G  the  /  to 
that  of  the  subdominant. 

Hence  chords  of  the  Seventh  come  to  play  an  important  part  in  modem  music 
for  the  purpose  of  effecting  well-connected  and  yet  rapid  transitions  from  chord  to  f 
chord,  and  urging  them  forward  by  the  action  of  dissonances.    In  this  way  par- 
ticularly, transitions  to  the  compound  tone  of  the  subdominant  are  easily  effected. 

Thus,  for  example,  beginning  with  the  triad  g  -{•  bi  ^  d  vfe  can  not  merely 
pass  to  the  chord  of  C,  or  c  -h  ei  —  g,  but,  letting  g  remain  as  a  Seventh,  to  the  chord 
of  the  Seventh  a^  —  c  +  ei  —  g,  which  unites  the  two  chords  c  -f  e,  —  ^  and 
ai  —  c  -t-  fii,  and  then  immediately  pass  to  t^i  — /+  a,,  which  is  related  to  the 
latter  chord,  so  that  two  steps  bring  us  to  the  other  extremity  of  the  system  of  C 
major.  This  transition  also  gives  the  best  progression  for  the  Seventh  {g  in  the 
example),  because  it  has  been  prepared  in  the  previous  chord,  and  is  resolved  by 
descent  (to  /)  in  the  succeeding  chord.  If  we  tried  the  same  transition  backwards, 
we  should  have  to  obtain  the  Seventh  g  by  progression  from  aj  in  the  chord  of 
di  —/  +  «!,  and  then  be  compelled  to  introduce  the  c  of  the  chord  of  the  Seventh 
abruptly,  because  we  should  have  a  prohibited  succession  of  Fifths  {dx  +  ai  and 
c  +  ^)  if  we  tried  to  descend  from  di.  We  must  rather  obtain  c  by  a  leap  from/,  ^ 
because  aj  in  the  first  triad  must  furnish  both  the  a^  and  g  of  the  chord  of  the 
Seventh.  Thus  the  transition  to  the  dominant  is  by  no  means  easy,  fluent,  and 
natural ;  it  is  much  more  embarrassed  than  the  passage  to  the  subdominant.  Con- 
sequently the  regular  and  usual  progression  of  the  chord  of  the  Seventh  is  for 
its  Seventh  to  descend  to  the  triad  whose  Fifth  is  the  root  of  the  chord  of  the 
Seventh.  Supposing  we  denote  the  root  of  the  chord  of  the  Seventh  by  I,  its 
Third  by  III,  &c.,  a  falling  Seventh  will  lead  us  to  either  of  these  chords : 

i_ni-v-vn  and  i-in-v-vn 
I  -  IV  -  VI  I  -  m  -  VI 

Of  these  two  transitions,  the  first,  which  leads  to  a  chord  of  which  IV  is  tiie  root, 
10  the  liveliest,  because  it  introduces  a  chord  with  two  new  tones.    Tbe.other, 

Digitized  by  V^OOQIC 


IT 


358  CHORDAL  SEQUENCES.  pabt  m, 

which  leads  to  a  triad  having  VI  for  its  root,  introduces  only  one  new  tone.  Henoe 
the  first  is  regarded  as  the  principal  method  of  resolving  chords  of  the  Seventh. 
For  example : 

g  +  by  —  d   \  f  Ci  -  g  +  bi  —  d 

\        \/     /  \         \/      / 

g    ...    c  +  e\  Ci    ...    ai  —  c 

<5  +  «i  —  9  +  *i  bi  --  d  \  f  +  ai 

I    V  /  i    \/  / 

c    ...  /  +  ai  bi    ...    Ci  —  g 

The  descent  of  the  tone  VII  introduces  the  tone  YI.  In  the  first  case  this  is  the 
Third  of  the  new  triad,  and  in  the  second  its  root.*    But  it  may  be  its  Fifth : 

i-m-v-vn 


11  -IV  -  VI 


This,  however,  could  only  occur  naturally  in  the  two  chords : 
bi—d    I    f  +  ai    and    bi-^d    |    /  —  a't> 

\  V"  / 

c  +  ei  —  g 


because  the  two  chords  of  the  Seventh  represent  the  compound  tone  of  G,  and  the 
tonic  chord  estabHshes  the  bond  of  union  between  its  two  sections.  In  other  cases 
our  scheme  gives  so-called  false  cadences : 

(/  +  61  —  d   I  /    and    g  +  by  —  d  \  f 
a,  —  c  +  fij  a*p  +  c  —  e*p 

which  are  justified  (the  first  as  most  natural)  by  the  fact  that  either  c  +  «i  or  c  —  e'b 
belongs  to  the  chord  of  the  normal  resolution.  Eameau  therefore  justly  observes 
that  this  kind  of  resolution  is  only  permissible  when  the  IV  of  the  second  chord  is 
the  normal  Fourth  of  the  I  in  the  chord  of  the  Seventh. 

This  exhausts  the  resolutions  by  the  descent  of  the  Seventh.    Those  in  which 
it  remains  unchanged  take  place  according  to  the  schemes  : 


I-m-V-Vnt   and      I-IH-V-Vnt 

I 

vn 


ii^iV   -   VII  n  —  i^  — 


In  the  first  the  Seventh  becomes  the  root,  in  the  second  the  Third  of  the  new 
chord.    If  it  were  the  Fifth,  the  new  chord  would  coincide  with  part  of  the  chord 
%  of  the  Seventh : 

I   -  m  -  V  -  VII 

I        I       I        I 
vn  -  m  -  V  -  vn.§ 

*  [As  examples  of  the  second  method  have      triad  6.— <2  |  /,  itself  a  dissonanoe ;  and  in  the 
been  omitted  in  the  text,  take  third  tne  imperfect  minor  d  |  Z+Oj. — TVymm- 

lator.] 


g+b,...ei  e^-g 


-g  ...  c  g  +  ^-cti/,  C  + 6,-^  +  0, 

^Translator.]  X/       I     I    \/      I     ^ 

a,        d   f,     d  ...  g  +  b^ 


t  [Examples :  .  -  -^  -7*1 

\Y...)  \y...i  yX..d         v'rVi  rtTi' 

Here  in  the  first  example  we  obtain  the  major  /•••^'i— a  |  /       0|...6i— gf +  0, 

triad  /+a|~c;  in  the  second  the  diminished  — TransUUof. 

Digitized  by  VjOOQIC 


CHAP.  xvm.         CONSECUTIVE  FIFTHS  AND  OCTAVES.  359 

Tti  these  conneGtions  the  resolution  is  towards  the  dominant  side.  The  transition  is 
most  decisive  in  the  first,  where  the  Seventh  becomes  the  root.  These  resolutions 
are  on  the  whole  less  usual,  because  we  pass  more  easily  and  frequently  from 
chords  on  the  dominant  side  into  chords  of  the  Seventh  of  the  direct  system.  In 
tlae  chords  of  the  reverted  system  these  transitions  occur  more  frequently,  because 
tlieir  Sevenths  may  enter  by  ascent,  and  hence  we  avoid  the  sequences  of  Fifths, 
Tvliich  greatly  embarrass  the  transitions  from  a  triad  to  a  chord  of  the  Seventh  on 
its  dominant  side. 

As  to  the  transitions  from  one  chord  of  the  Seventh  to  another,  or  to  a  dissonant 
triad  of  the  direct  system  which  may  be  regarded  as  a  mutilated  chord  of  the 
Seventh,  all  these  matters  are  sufficiently  developed  in  the  ordinary  manuals  of 
Thorough  Bass,  and  offer  no  difficulties  that  would  justify  us  in  dwelling  upon 
tbem  here. 

On  the  other  hand,  we  have  to  say  a  few  words  on  certain  rules  respecting  the  IT 
progression  of  the  individual  parts  in  polyphonic  compositions.  Originally,  as  we 
liave  already  remarked,  all  these  parts  were  of  equal  importance,  and  had  usually 
to  repeat  the  same  melodic  figures  in  succepsion.  The  harmony  was  a  secondary 
consideration,  the  melodic  progression  of  the  individual  voices  was  the  principal 
matter.  Hence  it  was  necessary  to  take  care  that  each  voice  should  stand  out 
clear  and  distinct  from  all  the  others.  The  relation  between  the  importance  of 
harmony  and  melody  has  certainly  altered  essentially  in  modern  music  ;  the  former 
has  attained  a  much  higher  independent  significance.  But,  after  all,  perfection  of 
harmony  must  arise  from  the  simultaneous  performance  of  several  voices,  each  of 
-which  has  its  own  beautiful  and  clear  melodic  progression,  and  each  of  which 
therefore  moves  in  a  direction  that  the  hearer  has  no  difficulty  in  understanding. 

On  this  rests  the  prohibition  of  consecutive  Fifths  and  Octaves,  The  meaning 
of  this  prohibition  has  given  rise  to  much  disputation.  The  meaning  of  pro- 
hibiting consecutive  Octaves  has  been  made  clear  by  musical  practice.  In  poly-  % 
phonic  music  two  voices  which  lie  one  or  two  Octaves  apart,  are  forbidden  to 
move  forward  in  such  a  way  that  after  their  next  step  they  should  be  also  one  or 
two  Octaves  apart.  But  precisely  in  the  same  way,  two  voices  in  a  polyphonic 
piece  are  forbidden  to  go  on  in  unison  for  several  notes,  while  for  complete  musical 
compositions  it  is  not  forbidden  that  two  voices,  or  even  all  the  voices,  should 
proceed  in  Unisons  or  Octaves,  for  the  purpose  of  strengthening  the  melodic  pro- 
gression. It  is  clear  that  the  reason  of  this  rule  must  he  in  the  Umiting  the  wealth 
of  the  progression  of  parts  by  Unisons  and  Octaves.  This  is  allowable  when  it  is 
intentionally  introduced  for  a  whole  melodic  phrase,  but  it  is  not  suited  for  a  few 
notes  in  the  course  of  a  piece,  where  it  can  only  give  the  impression  of  reducing 
the  richness  of  the  harmony  by  an  unskilful  accident.  The  accompaniment  of  a 
lower  part  by  a  voice  singing  an  Octave  higher,  merely  strengthens  part  of  the 
compound  tone  of  the  lower  voice,  and  hence  where  variety  in  the  progression  of 
parts  is  important,  does  not  essentially  differ  from  a  Unison.  ^ 

Now  in  this  respect  the  nearest  to  an  Octave  are  the  Twelfth,  and  its  lower 
octave,  the  Fifth.  Hence,  then,  consecutive  Twelfths  and  consecutive  Fifths  par- 
take of  the  same  imperfection  as  consecutive  Octaves.  But  the  case  is  somewhat 
worse.  It  is  possible  to  accompany  a  whole  melody  in  Octaves  when  desirable, 
without  committing  any  error,  but  this  cannot  be  done  for  Fifths  and  Twelfths 
without  changing  the  key.  It  is  impossible  to  proceed  by  a  single  diatonic  step 
from  the  tonic  as  root  with  an  accompaniment  of  Fifths,  without  departing  from 
the  key.  In  G  major,  we  ascend  from  the  Fifth  c  ±  gr  to  the  Fifth  (i  ±  a,  but  a 
does  not  belong  to  the  scale,  which  requires  the  deeper  a^ ;  we  descend  to  fei±/,  J, 
and  there  is  no  /j  J  in  the  scale  at  all.  The  other  upward  steps  from  d  exclusive 
to  ai  can  of  course  be  accompanied  by  perfect  Fifths  in  the  scale,  as  ^i  ±61, 
/±c',  g'i^',  ai±Ci'.  It  is  therefore  impossible  to  use  the  Twelfth  consistently 
for  increasing  the  richness  of  the  tone.  But  again,  when  the  intervals  of  a  Twelfth 
or  Fifth  are  continued  for  a  few  steps  in  melodic  progression,  they  have  simply  the 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


36o  CONSECUTIVE  FIFTHS  AND  OCTAVES.  pabt  ra. 

effect  of  strengthening  the  root.  For  the  Twelfth  this  arises  from  its  directly 
corresponding  to  one  of  the  upper  partial  tones  of  the  root.  For  the  Fifth  c  ±  ^, 
the  c  and  g  are  the  two  first  upper  partials  of  the  combinational  tone  G,  which 
necessarily  accompanies  the  Fifth.  Hence  an  accompaniment  in  Fifths  above, 
when  it  occurs  isolatedly  in  the  midst  of  a  polyphonic  piece,  is  not  only  open  to  the 
charge  of  monotony,  but  cannot  be  consistently  carried  out.  It  should  therefore 
be  always  avoided. 

But  that  consecutive  Fifths  merely  infringe  the  laws  of  artistic  composition, 
and  are  not  disagreeable  to  the  natural  ear,  is  evident  from  the  simple  fact  that  all 
the  tones  of  our  voice,  and  those  of  most  instruments,  are  accompanied  by  Twelfths, 
and  that  our  whole  tonal  system  reposes  upon  that  fsuat  When  the  Fifths  are 
introduced  as  merely  mechanical  constituents  of  the  compound  tone,  they  are 
therefore  fully  justified.     So  in  the  mixture  stops  of  organs.    In  these  stops  the 

f  pipes  which  give  the  prime  tones  of  the  compound,  are  always  accompanied  by 
others  which  give  its  harmonics,  as  the  Octaves,  Twelfths  variously  repeated,  and 
even  the  higher  major  Thirds.  By  this  means  the  performer  is  able  to  compose  a 
tone  of  a  much  more  penetrating,  piercing  quaUty,  than  it  would  be  possible  to 
produce  by  the  simple  organ  pipes  with  their  relatively  weak  upper  partial  tones. 
It  is  only  by  such  means  that  an  organ  is  able  to  dominate  over  the  singing  of  a 
large  congregation.  Almost  all  musicians  have  blamed  an  accompaniment  of 
Fifths,  or  even  Thirds,  but  fortunately  have  not  been  able  to  effect  anything 
against  the  practice  of  organ-builders.  In  fa^t  the  mixture  stops  of  an  organ 
merely  reproduce  the  masses  of  tone  which  would  have  been  created  by  bowed 
instruments,  trombones,  and  trumpets,  if  they  had  executed  the  same  music.  It 
would  be  quite  different  if  we  collected  independent  parts,  from  each  of  which  we 
should  have  to  expect  an  independent  melodic  progression  in  the  tones  of  the 
scale.     Such  independent  parts  cannot  possibly  move  with  the  precision  of  a 

f  machine ;  they  would  soon  betray  their  independence  by  slight  mistakes,  and  we 
should  be  led  to  subject  them  to  the  laws  of  the  scale,  which,  as  we  have  seen, 
render  a  consistent  accompaniment  in  Fifths  impossible. 

The  prohibition  of  Fifths  and  Octaves  extends  also,  but  with  less  strictness,  to 
the  next  adjacent  consonant  intervals,  when  two  of  them  are  so  placed  as  to  form 
a  connected  group  of  upper  partials  in  a  compound  tone.    Thus  transitions  like 

d  ,,,  g  +  bi     to    c.,,/+(ii, 

are  rulad  by  musical  theorists  to  be  inferior  to  transitions  like 

bi  —  d* ...  g'    to    a,  —  c' .../. 

For  df  g,  by  are  the  third,  fourth,  and  fifth  partial  tones  of  the  compound  G^,  but 
61,  d\  g'  could  only  be  regarded  as  its  fifth,  sixth,  and  eighth.  Hence  the  first 
f  position  of  the  chord  expresses  a  single  compoimd  tone  much  more  decidedly  than 
the  second,  which  is  often  allowed  to  be  continued  through  long  passages,  when  of 
course  the  nature  of  the  Thirds  and  Fourths  varies. 

The  prohibition  of  consecutive  Fifths  was  perhaps  historically  a  reaction  against 
the  first  imperfect  attempts  at  polyphonic  music,  which  were  confined  to  an  ac- 
companiment in  Fourths  or  Fifths,  and  then,  like  all  reactions,  it  was  carried 
too  far,  in  a  barren  mechanical  period,  till  absolute  purity  from  consecutive  Fifths 
became  one  of  the  principal  characteristics  of  good  musical  composition.  Modem 
harmonists  agree  in  allowing  that  other  beauties  in  the  progression  of  parts  are 
not  to  be  rejected  because  they  introduce  consecutive  Fifths,  although  it  is  advisable 
to  avoid  them,  when  there  is  no  need  to  make  such  a  sacrifice. 

There  is  also  another  point  in  the  prohibition  of  Fifths  to  which  HauptmAnn 
has  drawn  attention.  We  are  not  tempted  to  use  consecutive  Fifths  when  we  pass 
from  one  consonant  triad  to  another  which  is  nearly  related  to  it,  because  other 
progressions  lie  nearer  at  hand.    Thus  we  pass  from  the  triad  of  G  major  to  the 

Digitized  by  VjOOQlC 


CHAP.  XVIII.  CONSECUTIVE  FIFTHS  AND  OCTAVES.  361 

four  related  triads  in  the  following  manner,  the  fundamental  bass  proceeding  by 
Thirds  or  Fifths : 

c  -\-  Bx  —  g  c  +  ^i  —  ^  c  +  ei-gr      and  c   +  «i  —  ^ 

to  c  +  64  ...  ai,     to  c  ...  /  +  ai,  to  B^,,.  ex  —  g^    to  Bx  ...  d  ...  g. 

Bat  when  the  fundamental  bass  proceeds  in  Seconds,  and  hence  does  not  pass 
to  a  directly  related  chord,  the  nearest  position  of  the  new  chord  is  certainly  one 
which  produces  consecutive  Fifths.    For  example  : 

^  +  61  —  d'  or  (/  +  61  -  d' 

to  a  —  c'  +  Cxt  to  /  +  ai  —  c 

In  such  cases,  then,  we  must  have  recourse  to  other  transitions  by  larger 
intervals,  as :  " 

g  -{■  hx  —  d/  or  ^  +  &i  —  d' 

to  ^1...  ax  —  c',  toai— c   ...  / 

which  avoid  consecutive  Fifths. 

Hence  when  the  chords  are  closely  connected  by  near  relationship  and  small 
distance  in  the  scale,  consecutive  Fifths  do  not  present  themselves.  When  they 
occur,  therefore,  they  are  always  signs  of  abrupt  chordal  transition,  and  it  is  then 
better  to  assimilate  the  progression  of  parts  to  that  which  spontaneously  arises  in 
the  case  of  related  chords. 

This  consideration  respecting  consecutive  Fifths,  which  was  emphasised  by 
Hauptmann,  appears  to  give  the  law  greater  importance.  That  it  is  not  the  only 
motive  for  the  prohibition  of  consecutive  Fifths  appears  from  the  fact,  that  the 
forbidden  sequence 

gr  +  61  -  d'    to    /  +  a^  —  </  ^ 

is  allowed,  when  the  chords  are  in  the  position 

hx^d'„.g    to    ax—  c'  .,.f, 

although  the  step  in  the  fundamental  bass  is  the  same. 

The  prohibition  of  so-called  hidden  Fifths  and  Octaves  has  been  added  on  to 
the  prohibition  of  consecutive  Fifths  and  Octaves,  at  least  for  the  two  extreme 
voices  of  a  composition  in  several  parts.  This  prohibition  forbids  the  lowest  and 
uppermost  voice  in  a  piece  to  proceed  by  direct  motion  [that  is,  both  parts  ascending 
or  both  parts  descending]  into  the  consonance  of  an  Octave  or  Fifth  (including 
Twelfth).  They  should  rather  come  into  such  a  consonance  by  contrary  motion 
(one  descending  and  the  other  ascending).  In  duets  this  would  also  hold  for  the 
unison.  The  meaning  of  this  law  must  certainly  be,  that  whenever  the  extreme 
voices  unite  to  form  the  partial  tones  of  a  compound,  they  ought  to  have  reached  ^ 
a  state  of  relative  rest.  It  must  be  conceded  that  the  equihbrium  will  be  more 
perfect  when  the  extreme  parts  of  the  whole  mass  of  tone  approach  their  junction 
from  opposite  sides,  than  when  the  centre  of  gravity,  so  to  speak,  of  the  sonorous 
mass  is  displaced  by  the  parallel  motion  of  the  extreme  voices,  and  these  voices 
catch  one  another  up  with  different  velocities.  But  where  the  motion  proceeds  in 
the  same  direction,  and  no  relative  rest  is  intended,  the  hidden  Fifths  are  also  not 
avoided,  as  in  the  usual  formulae : 


m 


e=ff 


in  which  the  g±.diB  reached  by  passages  involving  hidden  Fifths. 

Another  rule  in  the  progression  of  parts,  prohibiting  false  relations  ^  must  have 
had  its  origin  in  the  requirements  of  the  singer.    But  what  the  singer  finds  a 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


362  RESULTS  OP  THE  INVESTIGATION.  pabt  m. 

difficulty  in  hitting,  must  naturally  also  appear  an  unusual  and  forced  skip  to  the 
hearer.  By  fake  relations  is  meant  the  case  when  two  tones  in  consecutive 
chords,  which  belong  to  different  voices,  form  false  Octaves  or  fedse  Fifths.  For 
example,  if  one  voice  in  the  first  chord  sings  bi  and  another  voice  in  the  next 
chord  sings  5[>,  or  the  first  has  c  and  the  second  cjj^,  there  are  false  Octave  rela- 
tions. False  Fifth  relations  are  forbidden  for  the  extreme  voices  only.  Thus  in 
the  first  chord  the  bass  has  ^p  in  the  second  the  soprano  has  /,  or  conversely, 
where  bi  .,./  ia  a  false  Fifth.  The  meaning  of  this  rule  is,  probably,  that  the 
singer  would  find  it  difficult  to  hit  the  new  tone  which  is  not  in  the  scale,  if 
he  had  just  heard  the  next  nearest  tone  of  the  scale  given  by  another  singer. 
Similarly,  when  he  has  to  take  the  false  Fifth  of  a  tone  which  is  prominent  in 
present  harmony  as  lowest  or  highest.  There  is  therefore  a  certain  sense  in  the 
prohibition,  but  numerous  exceptions  have  arisen,  as  the  ear  of  modem  musicians, 
f  singers  and  hearers,  has  become  accustomed  to  bolder  combinations  and  livelier 
progressions.  All  these  rules  were  essentially  intended  for  the  old  ecclesiastieal 
music,  where  a  quiet,  gentle,  well-contrived,  and  well-adjusted  stream  of  sound 
was  aimed  at,  without  any  intentional  effort  or  disturbance  of  the  smoothest 
equihbrium.  Where  music  has  to  express  effort  and  excitement,  these  rules 
become  meaningless.  Hidden  Fifths  and  Octaves  and  even  false  relations  of 
Fifths  are  found  in  abundance  in  the  chorales  of  Sebastian  Bach,  who  is  other- 
wise so  strict  in  his  harmonies,  but  it  must  be  admitted  that  the  motion  of  his 
voices  is  much  more  powerfully  expressed  than  in  the  old  Italian  ecclesiastical 
music. 


CHAPTER  XIX. 

ESTHETIOAL  RELATIONS. 


Let  us  review  the  results  of  the  preceding  investigation. 

Compound  tones  of  a  certain  class  are  preferred  for  all  kinds  of  music,  melodic 
or  harmonic ;  and  are  almost  exclusively  employed  for  the  more  delicate  and 
artistic  development  of  music:  these  are  the  compound  tones  which  have  har- 
monic upper  partial  tones,  that  is  compound  tones  in  which  the  higher  partial  tones 
have  vibrational  numbers  which  are  integral  multiples  of  the  vibrational  number  of 
the  lowest  partial  tone,  or  prime.  For  a  good  musical  effect  we  require  a  certain 
moderate  degree  of  force  in  the  five  or  six  lowest  partial  tones,  and  a  low  degree 
of  force  in  the  higher  partial  tones. 

This  class  of  compound  tones  with  harmonic  upper  partials  is  objectively  dis- 
tinguished by  including  all  sonorous  motions  which  are  generated  by  a  mechanical 
f  process  that  continues  to  act  uniformly,  and  which  consequently  produce  a  uniform 
and  sustained  sensation.  In  the  first  rank  among  them  stand  the  compound  tones 
of  the  human  voice,  man*s  first  musical  instrument  in  time  and  value.  The  com- 
pound tones  of  all  wind  and  bowed  instruments  belong  to  this  class. 

Among  the  bodies  which  are  made  to  emit  tones  by  striking,  some,  as  strings, 
have  also  harmonic  upper  partials,  and  these  can  be  used  for  artistic  music. 

The  greater  number  of  the  rest,  as  membranes,  rods,  plates,  &c.,  have  inhar- 
monic upper  partial  tones,  and  only  such  of  them  as  have  not  very  strong  secondary 
tones  of  this  kind  can  be  singly  and  occasionally  employed  in  connection  with 
musical  instruments  proper. 

Although  sonorous  bodies  excited  by  blows  may  continue  to  sound  for  some 
time,  their  tones  do  not  proceed  with  uniform  force,  but  diminish  more  or  less 
slowly  and  die  away.  Constant  power  over  the  intensity  of  tone,  therefore,  which 
is  indispensable  for  expressive  performance,  can  only  be  attained  on  instruments 
of  the  first  kind,  which  can  be  maintained  in  a  state  of  excitement,  and  which 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


CHAP.  XIX.  RESULTS  OP  THE  INVESTIGATION.  363 

produce  only  harmonic  upper  partial  tones.     On  the  other  hand,  bodies  excited  by 
blo^w^s  have  a  peculiar  value  for  clearly  defining  the  rhythm. 

A  second  reason  for  preferring  compound  tones  with  harmonic  upper  partials 
is  subjective  and  conditioned  by  the  construction  of  our  ear.  In  the  ear  even 
every  simple  tone,  if  sufficiently  intense,  excites  feeble  sensations  of  harmonic 
npper  partials,  and  each  combination  of  several  simple  tones  generates  combina- 
tional tones,  as  I  have  explained  at  the  end  of  Chap.  VII.  (p.  157^1590).  A  single 
compound  tone  with  irrational  partials,  when  sounded  with  sufficient  force,  thus 
produces  the  sensation  of  dissonance,  and  simple  tones  acquire  in  the  ear  itself 
something  of  the  nature  of  composition  out  of  harmonic  upper  partial  tones. 

We  are  justified  in  assuming  that  historically  all  music  was  developed  from 
song.  Afterwards  the  power  of  producing  similar  melodic  effects  was  attained  by 
means  of  other  instruments,  which  had  a  quality  of  tone  compoimded  in  a  manner 
resembling  that  of  the  human  voice.  The  reason  why,  even  when  constructive  H 
art  was  most  advanced,  the  choice  of  musical  instruments  was  necessarily  limited 
to  those  which  produced  compound  tones  with  harmonic  upper  partials,  is  clear 
from  the  above  conditions. 

This  invariable  and  peculiar  selection  of  instruments  makes  us  perfectly  certain 
that  harmonic  upper  partials  have  from  all  time  played  an  essential  part  in  musical 
constructions,  not  merely  for  harmony,  as  the  second  part  of  this  book  shews,  but 
also  for  melody. 

Again,  we  can  at  any  moment  convince  ourselves  of  the  essential  importance 
of  upper  partial  tones  to  melody,  by  the  absence  of  all  expression  in  melodies 
executed  with  objectively  simple  tones,  as,  for  example,  those  of  wide-stopped 
organ  pipes,  for  which  the  harmonic  upper  partials  are  formed  only  subjectively 
and  weakly  in  the  ear. 

A  necessity  was  always  felt  for  music  of  all  kinds  to  proceed  by  certain  definite 
degrees  of  pitch ;  but  the  choice  of  these  degrees  was  long  unsettled.  To  distin-  ^ 
guish  small  differences  of  pitch  and  intonate  them  with  certainty,  requires  a  greater 
amount  of  technical  musical  power  and  cultivation  of  ear,  than  when  the  intervals 
are  larger.  Hence  among  almost  all  uncivilised  people  we  find  the  Semitones 
neglected,  and  only  the  larger  intervals  retained.  For  some  of  the  more  cultivated 
nations,  as  the  Chinese  and  Gaels,  a  scale  of  this  kind  has  become  established.* 

It  might  perhaps  have  seemed  most  simple  to  make  all  such  degrees  of  pitch 
of  equal  amount,  that  is,  equally  well  distinguishable  by  our  sensations.  Such  a 
graduation  is  possible  for  all  our  sensations,  as  Fechner  has  shewn  in  his  investi- 
gations on  psychophysical  laws.  We  find  such  graduations  used  for  the  divisions  of 
musical  rhythm,  and  "the  astronomers  use  them  in  reference  to  the  intensity  of 
light  in  determining  stellar  magnitudes.  Even  in  the  field  of  musical  pitch,  the 
modem  equally  tempered  chromatic  scale  presents  us  with  a  similar  graduation. 
But  although  in  certain  of  the  less  usual  Greek  scales  and  in  modem  Oriental 
music,  cases  occur  where  some  particular  small  intervals  have  been  divided  on  the  ^ 
principle  of  equal  graduations,  yet  there  seems  at  no  time  or  place  to  have  been  a 
system  of  music  in  which  melodies  constantly  moved  in  equal  degrees  of  pitch, 
but  smaller  and  larger  intervals  have  always  been  mixed  in  the  musical  scales 
in  a  way  that  must  appear  entirely  arbitrary  and  irregular  until  the  relationship  of 
compound  tones  is  taken  into  consideration.* 

On  the  contrary,  in  all  known  musical  systems  the  intervals  of  Octave  and 
Fifth  have  been  decisively  emphasised.  Their  difference  is  the  Fourth,  and  the 
difference  between  this  and  the  Fifth,  is  the  Pythagorean  major  Tone  8  :  9,  by 
which  (but  not  by  the  Fourth  or  Fifth)  the  Octave  might  be  approximatively 
divided. 

The  sole  renmants  that  I  can  find  in  modem  music  of  the  endeavour  some- 
times made  in  homophonic  music  to  introduce  degrees  depending  on  equality  of 
interval  and  not  on  relationship  of  tone,  are  the  chromatic  intercalated  notes,  and 
*  [See,  however,  App.  XX.  sect.  E.— Translator.] 

Digitized  by  V^OOQ IC 


364  RESULTS  OP  THE  INVESTIGATION.  pabt  in. 

the  leading  note  of  the  key  when  similaxly  ased.  But  this  is  always  a  Semitone 
(p.  352c),  an  interval  well  known  in  the  series  of  related  tones,  which,  owing  to 
its  smallness,  is  easily  measured  hy  the  sensation  of  its  difference,  even  in  places 
where  its  tonal  relationship  is  not  immediately  sensible. 

The  decisive  importance  acquired  by  the  Octave  and  Fifth  iu  all  musical  scales 
from  the  earliest  times  shews,  that  the  construction  of  scales  must  have  been 
originally  influenced  by  another  principle,  which  Anally  became  the  sole  regulator 
of  every  artistic  form  of  a  complete  scale.  This  is  the  principle  which  we  have 
termed  tonal  relationship, 

Belationship  in  the  first  degree  between  two  compound  tones  consists  in  their 
each  having  a  partial  tone  of  the  same  pitch. 

In  singing,  the  similarity  of  two  musical  tones  which  stand  in  the  relation  of 
Octave  or  Fifth  to  one  another,  must  have  been  very  soon  observed.  As  already 
%  remarked,  this  gives  also  the  Fourth,  which  has  itself  a  sufficiently  perceptible 
natural  relationship  to  have  been  remarked  independently.  To  discover  the  tonal 
similarity  of  the  major  Third  and  major  Sixth,  required  a  finer  cultivation  of  the 
musical  ear,  and  perhaps  also  peculiar  beauty  of  voice.  Even  yet  we  are  easily  led 
by  the  fomiliar  sharp  major  Thirds  of  equal  temperament,  to  endure  any  major 
Thirds  which  are  somewhat  too  sharp,  provided  they  occur  melodically  and  are 
not  sounded  together.  On  the  other  hand,  we  must  not  forget  that  the  rules  of 
•  Archytas  and  Abdul  Kadir,*  both  of  which  were  applicable  to  homophonic  musie 
only,  gave  a  preference  to  the  natural  major  Third,  although  its  introduction 
obliged  both  musicians  to  renounce  a  musical  system  so  theoretically  consistent 
and  invested  with  such  high  authority  as  that  of  Pythagoras. 

Hence  the  principle  of  tonal  relationship  did  not  at  all  times  exclusively  deter- 
mine the  construction  of  the  scale,  and  does  not  even  yet  determine  it  exclusively 
among  all  nations.  This  principle  must,  therefore,  be  regarded  to  some  extent  as 
%  &  freely  selected  principle  of  style,  as  I  have  endeavoured  to  shew  in  Chapter  XTTT. 
But,  on  the  other  hand,  the  art  of  music  in  Europe  was  historically  developed 
from  that  principle,  and  on  this  fact  depends  the  main  proof  that  it  was  really  as 
important  as  we  have  assumed  it  to  be.  The  preference  first  given  to  the  diatonic 
scale,  and  finally  the  exclusive  use  of  that  scale,  introduced  the  principle  of  tonal 
relationship  in  all  its  integrity  into  the  musical  scale.  Within  the  diatonic  scale 
various  methods  of  execution  were  possible,  and  these  generated  the  ancient  modes, 
which  had  equal  claims  to  attention  in  homophonic  song,  and  hence  stood  on  a 
level. 

But  the  principle  of  tonal  relationship  penetrated  far  deeper  in  its  harmonic 
than  it  did  in  its  melodic  form.  In  melodic  sequence  the  identity  of  two  partial 
tones  is  a  matter  of  memory,  but  when  the  notes  are  sounded  together  the  im- 
mediate sensible  impression  of  the  beats,  or  else  of  the  undisturbed  flow  of  sound 
forces  itself  on  the  hearer's  attention.  The  liveliness  of  melodic  and  harmonic 
f  impressions  differs  in  the  same  way  as  a  recollected  image  differs  from  the  actual 
impression  made  by  the  original.  As  an  immediate  consequence  arose  that  £Eur 
superior  sensibility  for  the  correctness  of  the  intervals  which  is  seen  in  the  har- 
monic union  of  tones,  and  which  admitted  of  being  developed  into  the  finest 
physical  methods  of  measurement. 

It  must  also  be  remembered  that  relationship  in  the  second  degree  can  in 
harmonic  music  be  reduced  to  audible  relationships  of  the  first  degree,  by  a  proper 
selection  of  the  fundamental  bass,  and  that  generally  more  distant  relationships 
can  easily  be  made  clearly  audible.  By  this  means,  notwithstanding  the  variety 
of  progression,  a  much  clearer  connection  of  all  parts  with  their  origin,  the  tonic, 
can  be  maintained  and  rendered  objectively  sensible  to  the  hearer.  It  cannot  be 
doubted  that  these  are  the  essential  foundations  of  the  great  breadth  and  wealth  of 
expression  which  modem  compositions  can  attain  without  losing  their  artistic  unity. 

•  [For  Archytas  of  Tarentnm,  about  b.o.      note  ^.^TranskUor.'] 
400,  see  p.  262c,  and  for  Abdolqadir,  see  p.  281, 

Digitized  by  VjOOQIC 


CHAP.  XIX.  RESULTS  OF  THE  INVESTIGATION.  365 

We  then  saw  that  the  requirements  of  harmonio  musio  reacted  in  a  peculiar 
manner  on  the  construction  of  scales ;  that  properly  speaking  only  one  of  the  old 
tonal  modes  (our  major  mode)  could  be  retained  unaltered,*  and  that  the  rest  after 
undergoing  peculiar  modifications  were  fused  into  our  minor  mode,  which,  though 
most  like  the  ancient  mode  of  the  minor  Third,  can  at  one  time  resemble  the  mode 
of  the  minor  Sixth,  and  at  another  time  that  of  the  minor  Seventh,  but  does  not 
perfectly  correspond  with  any  one  of  these. 

This  process  of  the  development  of  the  elements  of  our  modem  musical 
system  lasted  down  to  the  middle  of  the  last  century.  It  was  not  until  composers 
ventured  to  put  a  minor  chord  at  the  close  of  compositions  written  in  the  minor 
mode,  that  the  musical  feeling  of  European  musicians  and  hearers  can  be  admitted 
to  have  become  perfectly  and  surely  habituated  to  the  new  system.  The  minor 
chord  was  allowed  to  be  a  real,  although  obscured,  chord  of  its  tonic. 

Whether  this  admission  of  the  minor  chord  expressed  a  feeling  for  another  V 
mode  of  unifying  its  three  tones,  as  A.  von  Oettingen  f  has  assumed, — relying 
on  the  fwt  that  the  three  tones  c—e^\}'^g  have  a  common  upper  partial  gr", — 
must  be  left  to  future  experience  to  decide,  should  it  be  found  practicable  to  con- 
struct long  and  well-connected  musical  compositions  in  Oettingen's  phonic  system 
(this  is  the  name  which  he  gives  to  the  minor  system  which  he  has  theoretically 
developed,  and  which  is  essentially  different  from  the  historical  minor  mode).  At 
any  rate,  the  minor  mode  has  historically  developed  itself  as  a  compromise 
between  dififerent  kinds  of  claims.  Thus  it  is  only  major  triads  which  can  per- 
fectly indicate  the  compound  tone  of  the  tonic ;  minor  chords  contain  in  their 
Third  sm  element  which,  although  nearly  related  to  the  tonic  and  its  Fifth,  does 
not  thoroughly  fuse  with  them,  and  hence  in  their  final  cadence  they  do  not  so 
thoroughly  agree  with  the  principle  of  tonality  which  had  ruled  the  previous 
development  of  music.  I  have  endeavoured  to  make  it  probable  that  the  peculiar 
esthetic  expression  of  the  minor  mode  proceeded  partly  from  this  cause  and  partly  If 
from  the  heterogenous  combinational  tones  of  the  minor  chord. 

In  the  last  part  of  my  book,  I  have  endeavoured  to  shew  that  the  construction 
of  scales  and  of  harmonic  tissue  is  a  product  of  artistic  invention,  and  by  no 
means  famished  by  the  natural  formation  or  natural  function  of  our  ear,  as  it  has 
t)een  hitherto  most  generally  asserted.  Of  course  the  laws  of  the  natural  function 
of  our  ear  play  a  great  and  influential  part  in  this  result ;  these  laws  are,  as  it 
were,  the  building  stones  with  which  the  edifice  of  our  musical  system  has  been 

*  [But  see  Bupra,  p.  274,  note  ♦,  scale  i.—  in  the  theory  of  composition.    For  the  rest 

Translator.]  this  author  justifies  (p.  54)  the  assertion  I  have 

f  The  System  of  Harmony  Dually  Deve-  made  in  the  text  by  remarking : '  I  am  sorry 

loped,  Dorpat  and  Leipzig,  1866.    Herr  y.  Get-  to  say  that  I  am  unable  to  adduce  a  single 

tingen,  as  already  observed,  p.  308,  note  §,  example  from  the  whole  of  our  musical  litera- 

regards  the  minor  chord  as  representing  the  ture,  of  the  carrying  out  of  (v.  Oettingen's)  pure 

harmonic  undertones  of  its  Fifth,  and  hence  as  minor  mode  harmony  even  in  the  simplest 

standing  in  place  of  a  part  of  its  compound  tone.  manner.*    I  have  not  been  able  to  convince^ 

He  caUs  it  the  Aphonic 'chord,  as  opposed  to  the  myself  of  the  correctness  of  the  fact  adduced 

*  tonic  *  major  chord  which  stands  in  place  of  on  p.  xiii.  and  p.  6,  that  the  undertones  of  a 

the  upper  partials  of  its  root.    He  proceeds  to  tone  strongly  struck  on  the  piano  sound  when 

deduce  the  formation  of  the  minor  system  from  the  corresponding  dampers  are  raised.  Perhaps 

the  relations  of  the  harmonic  undertones  in  a  the  author  has  been  deceived  by  the  circum- 

manner  precisely  analogous  to  that  by  which  stance  that  with  very  resonant  instruments 

I  have  deduced  the  major  system  from  the  (especiaUy  older  ones)  any  strong  shake,  and 

relations   of    the  upper  partial  tones.     The  therefore  probably  a  violent  blow  on  the  digitals, 

tonal  mode  thus  constructed  is,  however,  in  our  will  cause  some  one  or  several  of  the  deeper 

language  the  mode  of  the  minor  Sixth  (p.  274,  strings  to  sound  its  note.    [The  undertones 

note  *,  scale  7),  and  the  usual  minor,  a  mixed  have  always  each  an  upper  partial  tone  of  the 

mode.    Latterly  Dr.  Hugo  Biemann  has  given  pitch  of  the  note  struck ;  the  striking  of  this 

in  his  adhesion  to  this  view,  and  in  his  lately  note  must  then  sympathetically  excite  those 

published  Musical  Syniaxis  has  attempted  to  upper  partials  of  the  undertones,  and  thus 

examine  and  establish  the  consequences  of  reinforce  the  prime  of  the  note  struck,  just  as 

this  system  by  examples  from  acknowledged  striking  the  undertone  sympatheticaUy  excites 

composers.    The  application  of  this  critical  the  higher  tone  itself.    Can  this  have  deceived 

method  appears  to  me  very  commendable,  and  Dr.  Biemann  ? — Translator,] 
to  be  the  indispensable  condition  to  advancing 


Digitized  by 


Google 


366  ESTHETIC  ANALYSIS  OF  WORKS  OP  ART.  past  m. 

erected,  and  the  necessity  of  accurately  understanding  the  nature  of  these 
materials  in  order  to  understand  the  construction  of  the  edifice  itself,  has  been 
clearly  shewn  by  the  course  of  our  investigations  upon  this  very  subject.  But 
just  as  people  with  differently  directed  tastes  can  erect  extremely  different  kinds  of 
buildings  with  the  same  stones,  so  also  the  history  of  music  shews  us  that  the 
same  properties  of  the  human  ear  could  serve  as  the  foundation  of  very  different 
musical  systems.  Consequently  it  seems  to  me  that  we  cannot  doubt,  that  not 
merely  the  composition  of  perfect  musical  works  of  art,  but  even  the  construction 
of  our  system  of  scales,  keys,  chords,  in  short  of  all  that  is  usually  comprehended 
in  a  treatise  on  Thorough  Bass,  is  the  work  of  artistic  invention,  and  hence  most 
be  subject  to  the  laws  of  artistic  beauty.  In  point  of  fact,  mankind  has  been  at 
work  on  the  diatonic  system  for  more  than  2500  years  since  the  days  of  Terpander 
and  Pythagoras,  and  in  many  cases  we  are  still  able  to  determine  that  the  pro* 

«|[  gressive  changes  made  in  the  tonal  system  have  been  due  to  the  most  distin- 
guished composers  themselves,  partly  through  their  own  independent  inventions, 
and  partly  through  the  sanction  which  they  gave  to  the  inventions  of  others,  by 
employing  them  artistically. 

The  esthetic  analysis  of  complete  musical  works  of  art,  and  the  comprehension 
of  the  reasons  of  their  beauty,  encounter  apparently  invincible  obstacles  at  almost 
every  point.  But  in  the  field  of  elementary  musical  art  we  have  now  gained  so 
much  insight  into  its  internal  connection  that  we  are  able  to  bring  the  results  of 
our  investigations  to  bear  on  the  views  which  have  been  formed  and  in  modem 
times  nearly  universally  accepted  respecting  the  cause  and  character  of  artistic 
beauty  in  general.  It  is,  in  fact,  not  difBcult  to  discover  a  close  connection  and 
agreement  between  them ;  nay,  there  are  probably  fewer  examples  more  suitable 
than  the  theory  of  musical  scales  and  harmony,  to  illustrate  the  darkest  and  most 
dif&cult  points  of  general  esthetics.    Hence  I  feel  that  I  should  not  be  justified  in 

^  passing  over  these  considerations,  more  especially  as  they  are  closely  connected 
with  the  theory  of  sensual  perception,  and  hence  with  physiology  in  general. 

No  doubt  is  now  entertained  that  beauty  is  subject  to  laws  and  rules  dependent 
on  the  nature  of  human  inteUigence.  The  dif&culty  consists  in  the  forct  that  these 
laws  and  rules,  on  whose  fulfilment  beauty  depends  and  by  which  it  must  be  judged, 
are  not  consciously  present  to  the  mind,  either  of  the  artist  who  creates  the  work, 
or  the  observer  who  contemplates  it.  Art  works  with  design,  but  the  work  of  art 
ought  to  have  the  appearance  of  being  undesigned,  and  must  be  judged  on  that 
ground.  Art  creates  as  imagination  pictures,  regularly  without  conscious  law, 
designedly  without  conscious  aim.  A  work,  known  and  ackno\^ledged  as  the  pro- 
duct of  mere  intelligence,  will  never  be  accepted  as  a  work  of  art,  however  peifect 
be  its  adaptation  to  its  end.  Whenever  we  see  that  conscious  reflection  has  acted 
in  the  arrangement  of  the  whole,  we  find  it  poor. 

Man  fiihlt  die  Absioht,  and  man  wird  verstimmt. 
IT  (We  feel  the  purpose,  and  it  jars  upon  as.) 

And  yet  we  require  every  work  of  art  to  be  reasonable,  and  we  shew  this  by 
subjecting  it  to  a  critical  examination,  and  by  seeking  to  enhance  our  enjoyment  and 
our  interest  in  it  by  tracing  out  the  suitability,  connection,  and  equilibrium  of  all  its 
separate  parts.  The  more  we  succeed  in  making  the  harmony  and  beauty  of  all 
its  peculiarities  clear  and  distinct,  the  richer  we  find  it,  and  we  even  regard  as 
the  principal  characteristic  of  a  great  work  of  art  that  deeper  thought,  reiterated 
observation,  and  continued  reflection  shew  us  more  and  more  clearly  the  reason- 
ableness of  all  its  individual  parts.  Our  endeavour  to  comprehend  the  beauty  of 
such  a  work  by  critical  examination,  in  which  we  partly  succeed,  shews  that  we 
assume  a  certain  adaptation  to  reason  in  works  of  art,  which  may  possibly  rise  to 
a  conscious  understanding,  although  such  imderstanding  is  neither  necessary  for  the 
invention  nor  for  the  enjoyment  of  the  beautiful.  For  what  is  esthetically  beau> 
tiful  is  recognised  by  the  immediate  judgment  of  a  cultivated  taste,  which  declaims 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


CHAP.  XIX.        ESTHETIC  ANALYSIS  OF  WORKS  OF  ART.  367 

it  pleasing  or  displeasing,  without  any  comparison  whatever  with  law  or  concep- 
tion. 

But  that  we  do  not  accept  delight  in  the  beautiful  as  something  individual,  but 
rather  hold  it  to  be  in  regular  accordance  with  the  nature  of  mind  in  general, 
appears  by  our  expecting  and  requiring  from  every  other  healthy  human  intellect 
the  same  homage  that  we  ourselves  pay  to  what  we  call  beautiful.  At  most  we 
allow  that  national  or  individual  peculiarities  of  taste  incline  to  this  or  that  artistic 
ideal,  and  are  most  easily  moved  by  it,  precisely  in  the  same  way  that  a  certain 
amount  of  education  and  practice  in  the  contemplation  of  fine  works  of  art  is 
undeniably  necessary  for  penetration  into  their  deeper  meaning. 

The  principal  difficulty  in  pursuing  this  object,  is  to  understand  how  regularity 
can  be  apprehended  by  intuition  without  being  consciously  felt  to  exist.  And  this 
unconsciousness  of  regularity  is  not  a  mere  accident  in  the  effect  of  the  beautiful 
on  our  mind,  which  may  indifferently  exist  or  not ;  it  is,  on  the  contrary,  most  ^ 
clearly,  prominently,  and  essentially  important.  For  through  apprehending  every- 
where traces  of  regularity,  connection,  and  order,  without  being  able  to  grasp  the 
law  and  plan  of  the  whole,  there  arises  in  our  mind  a  feeling  that  the  work  of  art 
-which  we  are  contemplating  is  the  product  of  a  design  which  far  exceeds  anything 
we  can  conceive  at  the  moment,  and  which  hence  partakes  of  the  character  of  the 
illimitable.    Remembering  the  poet's  words : 

Da  gleichst  dem  Gteist,  den  da  begreifst, 
(Thoa*rt  like  the  spirit  thoa  oonceivest), 

we  feel  that  those  intellectual  powers  which  were  at  work  in  the  artist,  are  far  above 
our  conscious  mental  action,  and  that  were  it  even  possible  at  all,  infinite  time, 
meditation,  and  labour  would  have  been  necessary  to  attain  by  conscious  thought 
that  degree  of  order,  connection,  and  equilibrium  of  all  parts  and  all  internal 
relations,  which  the  artist  has  accomplished  under  the  sole  guidance  of  tact  and  ^ 
taste,  and  which  we  have  in  turn  to  appreciate  and  comprehend  by  our  own  tact  and 
taste,  long  before  we  begin  a  critical  analysis  of  the  work. 

It  is  clear  that  all  high  appreciation  of  the  artist  and  his  work  reposes  essen- 
tially on  this  feeling.  In  the  first  we  honour  a  genius,  a  spark  of  divine  creative 
fire,  which  hx  transcends  the  limits  of  our  intelligent  and  conscious  forecast.  And 
yet  the  artist  is  a  man  as  we  are,  in  whom  work  the  same  mental  powers  as  in  our- 
selves, only  in  their  own  peculiar  direction,  purer,  brighter,  steadier ;  and  by  the 
greater  or  less  readiness  and  completeness  with  which  we  grasp  the  artist's  language 
we  measure  our  own  share  of  those  powers  which  produced  the  wonder. 

Herein  is  manifestly  the  cause  of  that  moral  elevation  and  feeling  of  ecstatic 
satisfaction  which  is  called  forth  by  thorough  absorption  in  genuine  and  lofty  works 
of  art.  We  learn  from  them  to  feel  that  even  in  the  obscure  depths  of  a  healthy 
and  harmoniously  developed  human  mind,  which  are  at  least  for  the  present 
inaccessible  to  analysis  by  conscious  thought,  there  slumbers  a  germ  of  order  that  ^ 
is  capable  of  rich  intellectual  cultivation,  and  we  learn  to  recognise  and  admire  in 
the  work  of  art,  though  draughted  in  unimportant  material,  the  picture  of  a  similar 
arrangement  of  the  universe,  governed  by  law  and  reason  in  all  its  parts.  The 
contemplation  of  a  real  work  of  art  awakens  our  confidence  in  the  originally  healthy 
nature  of  the  human  mind,  when  uncribbed,  unharassed,  unobscured,  and  un- 
falsified. 

But  for  all  this  it  is  an  essential  condition  that  the  whole  extent  of  the  regularity 
and  design  of  a  work  of  art  should  no^  be  apprehended  consciously.  It  is  precisely 
from  that  part  of  its  regular  subjection  to  reason,  which  escapes  our  conscious 
apprehension,  that  a  work  of  art  exalts  and  delights  us,  and  that  the  chief  effects 
of  the  artistically  beautiful  proceed,  not  from  the  part  which  we  are  able  fully  to 
analyse. 

If  we  now  apply  these  considerations  to  the  system  of  musical  tones  and  har- 
mony, we  see  of  course  that  these  are  objects  belonging  to  an  entirely  subordinate 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


368  APPLICATION  TO  MUSIC.  pabt  nn 

and  elementary  domain,  but  nevertheless  they,  too,  are  slowly  matured  inventions 
of  the  artistic  taste  of  musicians,  and  consequently  they,  too,  must  be  governed  by 
the  general  rules  of  artistic  beauty.  Precisely  because  we  are  here  still  treading 
the  lower  walks  of  art,  and  are  not  dealing  with  the  expression  of  deep  psycho- 
logical problems,  we  are  able  to  discover  a  comparatively  simple  and  transparent 
solution  of  that  fundamental  enigma  of  esthetics. 

The  whole  of  the  last  part  of  this  book  has  explained  how  musicians  gradually 
discovered  the  relationships  between  tones  and  chords,  and  how  the  invention  of 
harmonic  music  rendered  these  relationships  closer,  and  clearer,  and  richer.  We 
have  been  able  to  deduce  the  whole  system  of  rules  which  constitute  Thorough 
Bass,  from  an  endeavour  to  introduce  a  clearly  sensible  connection  into  the  series 
of  tones  which  form  a  piece  of  music. 

A  feeling  for  the  melodic  relationship  of  consecutive  tones,  was  first  developed, 

^  commencing  with  Octave  and  Fifth  and  advancing  to  the  Third,  We  have  taJ^en 
pains  to  prove  that  this  feeling  of  relationship  was  founded  on  the  perception  of 
identical  partial  tones  in  the  corresponding  compound  tones.  Now  these  partial 
tones  are  of  course  present  in  the  sensations  excited  in  our  auditory  apparatus,  and 
yet  they  are  not  generally  the  subject  of  conscious  perception  as  independent  sensa- 
tions. The  conscious  perception  of  everyday  life  is  limited  to  the  apprehension  of 
the  tone  compounded  of  these  partials,  as  a  whole,  just  as  we  apprehend  the  taste 
of  a  very  compound  dish  as  a  whole,  without  clearly  feeling  how  much  of  it  is  due 
to  the  salt,  or  the  pepper,  or  other  spices  and  condiments.  A  critical  examination 
of  our  auditory  sensations  as  such  was  required  before  we  could  discover  the  exist- 
ence of  upper  partial  tones.  Hence  the  real  reason  of  the  melodic  relationship  of 
two  tones  (with  the  exception  of  a  few  more  or  less  clearly  expressed  conjectures, 
as,  for  example,  by  Bameau  and  d'Alembert)  remained  so  long  undiscovered,  or  at 
least  was  not  in  any  respect  clearly  and  definitely  formulated.    I  believe  that  I  have 

^  been  able  to  furnish  the  required  explanation,  and  hence  clearly  to  exhibit  the 
whole  connection  of  the  phenomena.  The  esthetic  problem  is  thus  referred  to  the 
common  property  of  all  sensual  perceptions,  namely,  the  apprehension  of  compound 
aggregates  of  sensations  as  sensible  symbols  of  simple  external  objects,  without 
analysing  them.  In  our  usual  observations  on  external  nature  our  attention  is  so 
thoroughly  engaged  by  external  objects  that  we  are  entirely  unpractised  in  taking 
for  the  subjects  of  conscious  observation,  any  properties  of  our  sensations  them- 
selves, which  we  do  not  already  know  as  the  sensible  expression  of  some  individual 
external  object  or  event. 

After  musicians  had  long  been  content  with  the  melodic  relationship  of  tones, 
they  began  in  the  middle  ages  to  make  use  of  harmonic  relationship  as  shewn  in 
consonance.  The  effects  of  various  combinations  of  tones  also  depend  partly  on 
the  identity  or  difference  of  two  of  their  different  partial  tones,  but  they  likewise 
partly  depend  on  their  combinational  tones.     Whereas,   however,   in  melodic 

-r  relationship  the  equahty  of  the  upper  partial  tones  can  only  be  perceived  by 
remembering  the  precedLag  compound  tone,  in  harmonic  relationship  it  is  deter- 
mined by  immediate  sensation,  by  the  presence  or  absence  of  beats.  Hence  in 
harmonic  combinations  of  tone,  tonal  relationship  is  felt  with  that  greater  liveli- 
ness due  to  a  present  sensation  as  compared  with  the  recollection  of  a  past  sensa- 
tion. The  wealth  of  clearly  perceptible  relations  grows  with  the  number  of  tones 
combined.  Beats  are  easy  to  recognise  as  such  when  they  occur  slowly ;  but  those 
which  characterise  dissonances  are,  almost  without  exception,  very  rapid,  and  are 
partly  covered  by  sustained  tones  which  do  not  beat,  so  that  a  careful  comparison 
of  slower  and  quicker  beats  is  necessary  to  gain  the  conviction  that  the  essence  of 
dissonance  consists  precisely  in  rapid  beats.  Slow  beats  do  not  create  the  feeling 
of  dissonance,  which  does  not  arise  till  the  rapidity  of  the  beats  confuses  the  ear 
and  makes  it  unable  to  distinguish  them.  In  this  case  also  the  ear  feels  the  dif- 
ference between  the  undisturbed  combination  of  sound  in  the  case  of  two  consonant 
tones,  and  the  disturbed  rough  combination  resulting  from  a  dissonance.    But,  as 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


CHAP.  XIX.       UNCONSCIOUS  SENSE  OF  RESEMBLANCE.  369 

s,  general  rule,  the  hearer  is  then  perfectly  unconscious  of  the  cause  to  which  the 
disturbance  and  roughness  are  due. 

The  development  of  harmony  gave  rise  to  a  much  richer  opening  out  of  musical  - 
art  than  was  previously  possible,  because  the  far  clearer  characterisation  of  related 
combinations  of  tones  by  means  of  chords  and  chordal  sequences,  allowed  of  tlie 
use  of  much  more  distant  relationships  than  were  previously  available,  by  modu- 
lating into  different  keys.  In  this  way  the  means  of  expression  greatly  increased 
as  well  as  the  rapidity  of  the  melodic  and  harmonic  transitions  which  could  now 
be  introduced  without  destroying  the  musical  connection. 

As  the  independent  significance  of  chords  came  to  be  appreciated  in  the  fifteenth 
and  sixteenth  centuries,  a  feeling  arose  for  the  relationship  of  chords  to  one  another 
and  to  the  tonic  chord,  in  accordance  with  the  same  law  which  had  long  ago 
unconsciously  regulated  the  relationship  of  compound  tones.  The  relationship  of 
compound  tones  depended  on  the  identity  of  two  or  more  partial  tones,  that  of  ^ 
chords  on  the  identity  of  two  or  more  notes.  For  the  musician,  of  course,  the  law 
of  the  relationship  of  chords  and  keys  is  much  more  intelligible  than  that  of  com- 
pound tones.  He  readily  hears  the  identical  tones,  or  sees  them  in  the  notes  before 
him.  But  the  unprejudiced  and  uninstructed  hearer  is  as  little  conscious  of  the 
reason  of  the  connection  of  a  clear  and  agreeable  series  of  fluent  chords,  as  he  is 
of  the  reason  of  a  well-connected  melody.  He  is  startled  by  a  false  cadence  and 
feels  its  unexpectedness,  but  is  not  at  all  necessarily  conscious  of  the  reason  of  its 
unexpectedness. 

Then,  again,  we  have  seen  that  the  reason  why  a  chord  in  music  appears  to  be 
the  chord  of  a  determinate  root,  depends  as  before  upon  the  analysis  of  a  com- 
pound tone  into  its  partial  tones,  that  is,  as  before  upon  those  elements  of  a 
sensation  which  cannot  readily  become  subjects  of  conscious  perception.  This  rela- 
tion between  chords  is  of  great  importance,  both  in  the  relation  of  the  tonic  chord 
to  the  tonic  tone,  and  in  the  sequence  of  chords.  % 

The  recognition  of  these  resemblances  between  compound  tones  and  between 
chords,  reminds  us  of  other  exactly  analogous  circumstances  which  we  must  have 
often  experienced.  We  recognise  the  resemblance  between  the  faces  of  two  near 
relations,  without  being  at  all  able  to  say  in  what  the  resemblance  consists, 
especially  when  age  and  sex  are  different,  and  the  coarser  outlines  of  the  features 
consequently  present  striking  differences.  And  yet  notwithstanding  these  differ- 
ences— notwithstanding  that  we  are  unable  to  fix  upon  a  single  point  in  the 
two  countenances  which  is  absolutely  alike — the  resemblance  is  often  so  extra- 
ordinarily striking  and  convincing,  that  we  have  not  a  moment's  doubt  about 
it.  Precisely  the  same  thing  occurs  in  recognising  the  relationship  between  two 
compound  tones. 

Again,  we  are  often  able  to  assert  with  perfect  certainty,  that  a  passage  not 
previously  heard  is  due  to  a  particular  author  or  composer  whose  other  works  we 
know.  Occasionally,  but  by  no  means  always,  individual  mannerisms  in  verbal  or  ^ 
musical  phrases  determine  our  judgment,  but  as  a  rule  we  are  mostly  unable  to  fix 
upon  the  exact  points  of  resemblance  between  the  new  piece  and  the  known  works 
of  the  author  or  composer. 

The  analogy  of  these  different  cases  may  be  even  carried  farther.  When  a 
father  and  daughter  are  strikingly  alike  in  sonie  well-marked  feature,  as  the  nose 
or  forehead,  we  observe  it  at  once,  and  think  no  more  about  it.  But  if  the  resem- 
blance is  so  enigmatically  concealed  that  we  cannot  detect  it,  we  are  fekscinated,  and 
cannot  help  continuing  to  compare  their  countenances.  And  if  a  painter  drew  two 
such  heads  having,  say,  a  somewhat  different  expression  of  character  combined 
-with  a  predominant  and  striking,  though  indefinable,  resemblance,  we  should 
undoubtedly  value  it  as  one  of  the  principal  beauties  of  his  painting.  Our  ad- 
miration would  certainly  not  be  due  merely  to  his  technical  skill ;  we  should 
rather  look  upon  his  painting  as  evidencing  an  unusually  delicate  feehng  for  the 

BB 

Digitized  by  VjOOQIC 


370  UNCONSCIOUS  SENSE   OP  TONAL  RELATIONSHIP,    pabt  hi. 

significance  of  the  human  countenance,  and  find  in  this  the  artistic  justification 
of  his  work. 

Now  the  case  is  similar  for  musical  intervals.  The  resemblance  of  an  Octave  to 
its  root  is  so  great  and  striking  that  the  dullest  ear  perceives  it ;  the  Octave  seems 
to  be  almost  a  pure  repetition  of  the  root,  as  it,  in  fact,  merely  repeats  a  part  of  the 
compound  tone  of  its  root,  without  adding  anything  new.  Hence  the  esthetical 
effect  of  an  Octave  is  that  of  a  perfectly  simple,  but  little  attractive  interval.  The 
most  attractive  of  the  intervals,  melodically  and  harmonically,  are  clearly  the 
Thirds  and  Sixths, — ^the  intervals  which  he  at  the  very  boundary  of  those  that  the 
ear  can  grasp.  The  major  Third  and  the  major  Sixth  cannot  be  properly  appre- 
ciated unless  the  first  five  partial  tones  are  audible.  These  are  present  in  good 
musical  qualities  of  tone.  The  minor  Third  and  the  minor  Sixth  are  for  the  most 
part  justifiable  only  as  inversions  of  the  former  intervals.    The  more  compHcated 

^  intervals  in  the  scale  cease  to  have  any  direct  or  easily  intelligible  relationship. 
They  have  no  longer  the  charm  of  the  Thirds. 

Moreover,  it  is  by  no  means  a  merely  external  indifferent  regularity  which  the 
employment  of  diatonic  scales,  founded  on  the  relationship  of  compound  tones,  has 
introduced  into  the  tonal  material  of  music,  as,  for  instance,  rhythm  introduced 
some  such  external  arrangement  into  the  words  of  poetry.  I  have  shewn,  on  the 
contrary,  in  Chapter  XIY.,  that  this  construction  of  the  scale  furnished  a  means  of 
measuring  the  intervals  of  their  tones,  so  that  the  equaUty  of  two  intervals  lying 
in  different  sections  of  the  scale  would  be  recognised  by  immediate  sensation. 
Thus  the  melodic  step  of  a  Fifth  is  always  characterised  by  having  the  second 
partial  tone  of  the  second  note  identical  with  the  third  of  the  first.  This  produces 
a  definiteness  and  certainty  in  the  measurement  of  intervals  for  our  sensation, 
such  as  might  be  looked  for  in  vain  in  the  system  of  colours,  otherwise  so 
similar,  or  in  the  estimation  of  mere  differences  of  intensity  in  our  various  sensual 

IT  perceptions. 

Upon  this  reposes  also  the  characteristic  resemblance  between  the  relations  of 
the  musical  scale  and  of  space,  a  resemblance  which  appears  to  me  of  vital  impor> 
tance  for  the  peculiar  effects  of  music.  It  is  an  essential  character  of  space  that 
at  every  position  vTithin  it  like  bodies  can  be  placed,  and  like  motions  can  occur. 
Everything  that  is  possible  to  happen  in  one  part  of  space  is  equally  possible  in 
every  other  part  of  space  and  is  perceived  by  us  in  precisely  the  same  way.  This 
is  the  case  also  with  the  musical  scale.  Every  melodic  phrase,  every  chord,  which 
can  be  executed  at  any  pitch,  can  be  also  executed  at  any  other  pitch  in  such  a  way 
that  we  immediately  perceive  the  characteristic  marks  of  their  similarity.  On  the 
other  hand,  also,  different  voices,  executing  the  same  or  different  melodic  phrases, 
can  move  at  the  same  time  within  the  compass  of  the  scale,  like  two  bodies  in 
space,  and,  provided  they  are  consonant  in  the  accented  parts  of  bars,  without 
creating  any  musical  disturbances.     Such  a  close  analogy  consequently  exists  in 

^  all  essential  relations  between  the  musical  scale  and  space,  that  even  alteration  of 
pitch  has  a  readily  recognised  and  unmistakable  resemblance  to  motion  in  space, 
and  is  often  metaphorically  termed  the  ascending  or  descending  motion  or  progres- 
sion of  a  part.  Hence,  again,  it  becomes  possible  for  motion  in  music  to  imitate 
the  peculiar  characteristics  of  motive  forces  in  space,  that  is,  to  form  an  image  of 
the  various  impulses  and  forces  which  lie  at  the  root  of  motion.  And  on  this,  as  I 
believe,  essentially  depends  the  power  of  music  to  picture  emotion. 

It  is  not  my  intention  to  deny  that  music  in  its  initial  state  and  simplest  forms 
may  have  been  originally  an  artistic  imitation  of  the  instinctive  modulations  of  the 
voice  that  correspond  to  various  conditions  of  the  feelings.  But  I  cannot  think  that 
this  is  opposed  to  the  above  explanation  ;  for  a  great  part  of  the  natural  means  of 
vocal  expression  may  be  reduced  to  such  facts  as  the  following :  its  rhythm  and 
accentuation  are  an  immediate  expression  of  the  rapidity  or  force  of  tiie  corre* 
spending  psychical  motives — all  effort  drives  the  voice  up — a  desire  to  make  a  plea- 
sant impression  on  another  mind  leads  to  selecting  a  softer,  pleasanter  quality  of 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


CHAP.  XIX.  EXPEESSION  OF  MOTION  IN  MUSIC.  371 

tone — and  so  forth.  An  endeavour  to  imitate  the  involuntary  modulations  of  the 
voice  and  make  its  recitation  richer  and  more  expressive,  may  therefore  very  pos- 
sibly have  led  our  ancestors  to  the  discovery  of  the  first  means  of  musical  expres- 
sion, just  as  the  imitation  of  weeping,  shouting,  or  sobbing,  and  other  musical 
delineations  may  play  a  part  in  even  cultivated  music,  (as  in  operas),  although 
such  modifications  of  the  voice  are  not  confined  to  the  action  of  free  mental 
motives,  but  embrace  really  mechanical  and  even  involuntary  muscular  contrac- 
tions. But  it  is  quite  clear  that  every  completely  developed  melody  goes  far  beyond 
an  imitation  of  nature,  even  if  we  include  the  cases  of  the  most  varied  alteration 
of  voice  under  the  influence  of  passion.  Nay,  the  very  fact  that  music  introduces 
progression  by  fixed  degrees  both  in  rhythm  and  in  the  scale,  renders  even  an 
approximativdy  correct  representation  of  nature  simply  impossible,  for  most  of 
the  passionate  affections  of  the  voice  are  characterised  by  a  ghding  transition  in 
pitch.  The  imitation  of  nature  is  thus  rendered  as  imperfect  as  the  imitation  of  IT 
a  picture  by  embroidery  on  a  canvas  with  separate  little  squares  for  each  shade  of  ' 
colour.  Music,  too,  departed  still  further  from  nature  when  it  introduced  the 
greater  compass,  the  mobility,  and  the  strange  qualities  of  tone  belonging  to  musical 
instruments,  by  which  the  field  of  attainable  musical  effects  has  become  so  much 
wider  than  it  was  or  could  be  when  the  human  voice  alone  was  employed. 

Hence  though  it  is  probably  correct  to  say  that  mankind,  in  historical  develop* 
ment,  first  learned  the  means  of  musical  expression  from  the  human  voice,  it  can 
hardly  be  denied  that  these  same  means  of  expressing  melodic  progression  act, 
in  artistically  developed  music,  without  the  slightest  reference  to  the  application 
made  of  them  in  the  modulations  of  the  human  voice,  and  have  a  more  general 
significance  than  any  that  can  be  attributed  to  innate  instinctive  cries.  That  this 
is  the  case  appears  above  all  in  the  modem  development  of  instrumental  music, 
which  possesses  an  effective  power  and  artistic  justification  that  need  not  be  gain- 
said, although  we  may  not  yet  be  able  to  explain  it  in  all  its  details.  V 


Here  I  close  my  work.  It  appears  to  me  that  I  have  carried  it  as  far  as  the 
physiological  properties  of  the  sensation  of  hearing  exercise  a  direct  influence  on 
the  construction  of  a  musical  system,  that  is,  as  &r  as  the  work  especially  belongs 
to  natural  philosophy.  For  even  if  I  could  not  avoid  mixing  up  esthetic  problems 
with  physical,  the  former  were  comparatively  simple,  and  the  latter  much  more 
complicated.  This  relation  would  necessarily  become  inverted  if  I  attempted  to 
proceed  further  into  the  esthetics  of  music,  and  to  enter  on  the  theory  of  rhythm, 
forms  of  composition,  and  means  of  musical  expression.  In  all  these  fields  the 
properties  of  sensual  perception  would  of  course  have  an  influence  at  times,  but  only 
in  a  very  subordinate  degree.  The  real  difficulty  would  lie  in  the  development  of 
the  psychical  motives  which  here  assert  themselves.  Certainly  this  is  the  point  IT 
where  the  more  interesting  part  of  musical  esthetics  begins,  the  aim  being  to  ex- 
plain the  wonders  of  great  works  of  art,  and  to  learn  the  utterances  and  actions  of 
the  various  affections  of  the  mind.  But,  however  alluring  such  an  aim  may  be,  I 
prefer  leaving  others  to  carry  out  such  investigations,  in  which  I  should  feel  myself 
too  much  of  an  amateur,  while  I  myself  remain  on  the  safe  ground  of  natural 
phdUosopby,  in  which  I  am  at  home* 


Digitized  by 


GaQgle 


APPENDICES- 


APPENDIX  I. 

«r  ON  AN  ELEGTBO-MAON^ITIC  DBIYIKG  MACHINB  FOB  THE  6IBEN. 

(See  p.  13a.) 

I  HAVE  lately  had  a  small  electro-magnetic  machine  constructed  with  a  constant 
velocity  of  rotation,  and  it  has  proved  of  great  service  in  driving  the  siren.  A 
rotating  electro-magnet,  in  which  the  direction  of  current  is  changed  every  semi* 
rotation,  moves  between  two  fixed  magnetic  poles.  The  current  in  this  electro- 
magnet is  interrupted,  as  soon  as  the  velocity  begins  to  exceed  the  desired  amount, 
by  the  centrifugal  force  of  a  mass  of  metal  febstened  to  its  axis  of  rotation.  Two 
spiral  springs  whose  elasticity  is  opposed  to  the  centrifugal  force,  maybe  tightened 
or  loosened,  and  thus  made  stronger  or  weaker  at  pleasure.  By  this  means  the 
velocity  can  be  maintained  at  any  required  rate.  A  figure  and  description  of  this 
machine  were  given  by  Herr  S.  Exner,  in  the  '  Proceedings '  {Sitzungsberichte) 
of  the  Vienna  Academy :  *  Math.  Naturw.  CI.*  vol.  Iviii.  part  2,  8  Oct.  1868. 

The  machine  was  improved  in  1875  by  separating  from  it  the  centrifugal 
ir  regulator,  and*  letting  it  only  open  and  close  the  weaJk  current  for  a  relay.  It 
is  the  relay  which  makes  or  breaks  the  strong  current  that  drives  the  electro- 
magnetic machine. 

The  siren  is  connected  with  the  machine  by  a  thin  driving  band,  and  then 
it  does  not  require  to  be  blown.  Instead  of  blowing,  I  placed  on  the  disc  a 
small  turbine  constructed  of  stiff  paper,  which  drove  the  air  through  the  open- 
ings whenever  they  coincided  with  those  in  the  chest.  This  arrangement  gave 
me  extremely  constant  tones  on  the  siren,  rivalling  those  on  the  best  constructed 
organ  pipes.  Latterly  I  have  given  the  siren  straight  holes,  so  that  the  strength 
of  the  wind  has  no  longer  any  influence  on  its  speed,  and  then  I  blow  through  the 
box.    [See  App.  XX.  sect.  B.  No.  2.] 


APPENDIX  H. 

ON  THE  SIZE  AND  CONSTRUCTION  OF  BBSONATOBS» 

(See  pp.  44b  and  i66d,  note  ♦.) 

Sphebioal  Resonators  with  a  short  fimnel-shaped  neck  for  insertion  into  the 
ear  as  shewn  in  fig.  16  a  (p.  4$b),  are  most  ^cient.  Their  advantage  consists 
partly  in  their  other  proper  tones  being  very  distant  indeed  from  their  prime  tones, 
and  nence  being  very  slightly  reinforced,  and  partly  in  the  spherical  form  giving 
the  most  powerful  resonance.  But  the  walls  of  the  sphere  must  be  firm  and 
smooth,  to  oppose  the  necessary  resistance  to  the  powerful  vibrations  of  air  whioh 
take  place  within  them,  and  to  impede  the  motion  of  the  air  as  litUe  as  possible 
by  firiction.  At  first  I  employed  any  spherical  glass  vessels  that  came  to  hand, 
as  the  receivers  of  retorts,  and  inserted  into  one  of  their  openings  a  glass  tube 
which  had  been  adapted  to  my  ear.  Afterwards  Herr  R.  Koenig,  maker  of  acous- 
tical instruments,  Paris  [now  of  27  Quai  d'Anjou],  constructed  a  series  of  these 

Digitized  by  VjOOQIC  ^ 


APP.  II. 


SIZE  AND  CONSTRUCTION  OF  EESONATORS. 


373 


glass  spheres  properly  tuned,  and  afterwards  had  them  made  of  brass  in  the  form 
shewn  in  fig.  i6  a,  p.  436.  This  is  the  most  appropriate  form  for  resonators. 
When  the  openings  are  relatively  very  narrow,  their  pitch  can  be  determined  by 
the  formula  which  I  have  developed,  namely* 


n 


where  a  is  the  velocity  of  sound,  a  the  area  of  the  circular  opeiung,  and  S  the 
volume  of  the  cavity*    Or  if  we  assume  as  its  value 

a  =  332*260  metres, 
which  corresponds  with  a  temperature  of  zero  centigrade,  the  above  formula  gives 

~ =56174  4^  (^)  , 

Herr  Sondhauss  [Pogg.  vol.  Ixxxi.  pp.  347-373]  had  discovered  the  same  formula 
experimentally,  but  used  52400  for  the  numerical  coefficient,  which  agrees  with 
the  experimental  results  better  when  the  openings  are  not  very  small.  When 
the  diameter  of  the  opening  is  smaller  than  one-tenth  of  the  diameter  of  the 
sphere,  the  formula  deduced  from  theory  agrees  well  with  Wertheim's  experiments. 
For  resonators  which  have  the  diameter  of  their  opening  between  a  fourth  and  a 
fifth  of  the  diameter  of  the  sphere,  I  have  experimentally  determined  the  coeffi- 
cient as  47000.  The  second  opening  of  the  resonator  may  be  regarded  as  closed 
because  it  is  brought  firmly  against  the  ear.  If  the  cavity  is  spherical  with  radius 
jB,  while  r  is  that  of  the  opening,  the  above  formula  becomes 


'-'■'  (rfs.) 


Here  follows  a  hst  of  the  measurements  of  my  glass  resonators. 


Fitoh 


I)    9 

r 

5) 

6) 


il 

d' 

8)  V\> 

9)  6"b 

10)   <r" 


% 


Diameter  of  the 

Sphere 

in  millimetres 

[and  inohes] 


131 
130 

115 
79 
76 
70 


[606] 

[5-i6] 

[5-12] 

[4-53] 
[3-1 1] 
[2-99] 

[276] 


53*5  [2-11] 


46 
43 


[r8i] 
[1-69] 


Diameter  of  the 

Oriflce 
in  millimetrea 
[and  inches] 


35'S  [i'4o] 
285  [I  1 2] 
302  [119] 

30     [118] 
"       [73] 


i8-5 
22 
205 
8 

IS 

IS 


[•87] 
[■81] 

[•31] 

[•S9] 
[•S9] 


Volumes  of  the 

Interior 

in  cubic  centimetres 

[and  cnbic  inches] 


1773  [108-19] 

1092  [66*64] 

1053  [6426] 

546  [33-32] 

23s  [14-341] 

214  [1306] 

162  [9-89] 

74  [452] 


49 
37 


[2-99] 

[2-26] 


Remarks 


Neck 
'funnel  shaped 


Neck  oylindrioal 

{Keck  cylindrical, 
month  at  side 
Neck  oylindrioal 


Smaller  spheres  did  not  answer  welL  In  order  to  tune  the  resonators,  Herr 
£oenig  has  also  made  them  of  two  short  cylinders  of  which  one  runs  into  tiie 
other,  each  having  a  pierced  lid  at  its  external  end.  One  opening  serves*  for  a 
connection  with  the  ear  or  a  sympathetic  flame,  the  other  is  free.f  The  measure- 
ments given  in  App.  IV.,  p.  377^^,  will  serve  for  the  manufibcture  of  such  tubes,  as 
the  second  opening  is  of  no  consequence  because  it  is  firmly  inserted  intOithe  ear. 

Since  metal  cubes  are  troublesome  to  manufacture  and  hence  proportionately 
dear,  we  can  use  double  cones  of  tin  or  pasteboard,  the  vertices  of  which  have 
•been  removed.  The  cone  next  the  ear  is  more  shfurply  poizited  so  that  its  end 
readily  fits  the  ear. 

Conical  resonators  of  thin  sheet  zinc,  such  as  HerrG.  Appunn  %  of  Hanau  manu- 


*  *  Theory  of  Vibrations  of  Air  in  Tubes 
with  Open  Ends/  in  Jourjial  fUr  reine  u,  an- 
gew.  Mathematikf  vol.  Ivii.,  equation  30a,  and 


following. 

t  Poggendorff's  Annals ^  vol.  ezlvi.  p.  189. 
X  [Deceased,  now  Anton  Appunn.^ 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


374  MOTION  OF  PLUCKED   STRINGS.  app.  ii.  in. 

factures,*  are  easily  made,  and  are  useful  for  most  purposes.  But  they  reinforce 
all  the  partials  of  their  fundamental  tone  at  the  same  time.  Their  length  is  about 
the  same  as  that  of  open  organ  pipes  of  the  same  pitch. 

Eesonators  with  a  very  narrow  opening  generally  produce  a  much  more  con- 
siderable reinforcement  of  the  tone,  but  then  there  must  be  a  much  more  precise 
agreement  between  the  pitch  of  the  tone  to  be  heard,  and  the  proper  tone  of  the 
resonator.  It  is  just  as  in  microscopes ;  the  greater  the  magnifymg  power,  the 
smaller  the  field  of  view.  Eeducing  the  size  of  the  orifice  also  deepens  the  pitch 
of  the  resonator,  and  this  gives  an  easy  means  of  tuning  it  to  any  required  pitch. 
But,  for  the  above  reason,  the  opening  must  not  be  reduced  too  much. 

I  should  also  mention  Herr  Koenig*s  plan  of  transferring  vibrations  of  air  to 
gas  flames,  and  thus  making  them  visible.  Flames  of  this  sort  act  well  when 
connected  with  resonators,  which  are  then  best  made  of  a  spherical  form,  and 
should  have  two  equal  openings.  To  one  of  the  openings  the  small  gas-chaonber 
is  fixed.  This  chamber  is  a  small  flat  box,  about  big  enough  to  contain  two 
5[  shilling  pieces  laid  flat  on  one  another.  It  is  cut  out  of  a  plate  of  wood,  and 
closed  on  the  side  next  the  resonator  with  a  very  thin  membrane  of  india-rubber, 
which,  wkLle  it  completely  separates  the  air  of  the  resonator  from  the  gas  in  the 
chamber,  allows  the  vibrations  of  the  air  to  be  freely  communicated  to  the  gas. 
Through  the  plate  of  wood  two  narrow  pipes  enter  the  chamber,  one  introducing 
the  inflammable  gas  and  the  other  conducting  it  away.  This  last  ends  in  a  Teiy 
fine  point  at  which  the  gas  is  lighted.  The  vibrations  of  the  air  in  the  re- 
sonator being  communicated  to  the  gas  cause  the  flame  to  leap  up  and  down. 
These  oscillations  of  the  flame  are  so  rapid  and  regular  that,  when  viewed  directly, 
the  flame  appears  to  be  quite  steady.  Its  altered  condition,  however,  betrays  itself 
by  an  altered  form  and  colour.  Thus  to  recognise  the  beats  of  two  tones  rein- 
forced by  the  resonator,  it  is  enough  to  look  at  the  flame  and  observe  how  it  alter- 
nates between  its  forms  of  rest  and  of  oscillation.  But  to  see  the  separate 
oscillations  the  flames  should  be  viewed  in  a  rotating  glass,  in  which  the  flame  at 
rest  appears  to  be  drawn  out  into  a  long  uniform  ribbon,  while  the  oscillating  flame 
f  appears  as  a  series  of  separate  images  of  flames.  It  is  thus  possible  to  allow  a 
large  niunber  of  persons  at  once  to  determine  whether  or  not  a  given  tone  is 
reinforced  by  the  resonator .f 

An  extremely  sensitive  means  of  making  the  vibrations  of  the  air  in  a  resonator 
visible,  is  a  flat  film  of  glycerined  soap  and  water  which  is  placed  over  its  opening. 

[Mr.  Bosanquet  finding  that  for  observations  on  beats  all  these  resonators  im- 
perfectly plug  one  ear  and  leave  the  other  open,  has  invented  another  kind,  for 
which  see  App.  XX.  sect.  L.  art.  4,  b.] 


APPENDIX  in. 

ON  THE  MOTION  OF  PLUCKED  STBINGS. 

(See  p.  526.) 


Let  X  be  the  distance  of  a  point  in  the  string  from  one  of  its  extremities,  and  I 
the  length  of  the  string,  so  that  for  one  extremity  a?=o,  and  for  the  other  a;=Z. 
It  is  suflicient  to  investigate  the  case  for  which  the  motion  takes  place  in  one 

Elane  passing  through  the  position  of  rest.    Let  y  be  the  distance  of  the  point  x 
:om  its  position  of  rest  at  the  time  t    And  let  f(  be  the  weight  of  the  unit 

*  [There  is  a  set  in  the  Soienoe  Collections  octaves  were  sang  to  the  French  vowels,  were 

at  South  Kensington  Museum. — Translator.]  also  exhibited.     See  Koenig's  paper  on   the 

t  [All  these  instruments  and  appliances  subject,  with  plates,  in  Philosophical  Maga- 

can  be  obtained  of  Herr  Eoenig,  by  whom  ^tm,    1873,   vol,  xlv.  pp,  1-18,  105-14. — On 

they  were  exhibited  in  London  at  the  Inter-  the  principles  of  the  use  of  revolving  mirrors, 

national  Exhibition  of  1872.    Large  drawings  first  experimentally  used  by  Sir  C.  Wheats 

of  the  appearances  of  the  flames  just  described,  stone,  see  Donjcin's  Acoustics,  1870,  p.  142.— 

as  viewed  in  the  rotating  mirror  while  two  Translator*] 

Digitized  by  V^OOQIC 


APP.  in.  MOTION  OF  PLUCKED  8TBINGS.  375 

of  length,  and  T  the  tensiou  of  the  string.    The  differential  equation  of  motion  is 
then 

'"'w   -^'W ^' 

Then,  since  the  extremities  of  the  string  are  assumed  to  be  at  rest,  we  must  have 
^  =  o  when  rcsro,  and  also  when  a;  =  {  (la) 

The  most  general  integral  of  the  equation  (i)  which  fulfils  the  conditions  (la) 
and  corresponds  to  a  periodic  motion  of  the  string,  is 

y  =  ill .  sin  ^ .  cos  ^^n^  +  ilj .  sin  ?^.  cos  ^ifnt^ 


+  ils  .  sin  3^  .  cos  6  nnt  +  &c. 

+  Si .  sin  !!^ ,  sin  2imt  +^2  •  sin  ^ .  sin  4'tmt 

+  ^3  .  sin  5^ .  sin  Simt  +  &c. 


(lb) 


T 
where  n*  =  — =.  and  Ai,  A^,  A^,  &o.,  and  Bi,  B^,  B^,  &c.,  are  any  constant  co« 
4/1 1* 

efficients,  which  can  be  determined  when  the  form  and  velocity  of  the  string  aro 

known  for  any  determinate  time  t. 

For  t  ss  o,  the  form  of  the  string  will  be 

y  =  4,.flin^+^.55i^+4,.Bin2p  +  &c (ic) 

and  the  velocity  of  the  string  will  be 

g  =  a«»(B,.sin!:^  +  2S,.8in?^  +  3B,.8in3p  +  &c.)...    (id)     ^ 

Now  suppose  the  string  to  have  been  drawn  aside  by  a  sharp  point,  and  that 
the  point  was  withdrawn  at  the  time  ^  =  o,  so  that  the  vibration  commenced  at 

that  moment,  then  for  t=o  there  was  no  velocity,  that  is  -^  was  =  o  for  all 

^values  of  ic.  This  can  only  be  the  case  when  in  equation  (id),  o^Bi  ^B^  s=58=&c. 
The  coefficients  A^yA^^A^^  &c.,  depend  on  the  shape  of  the  string  at  the  time 
^  =:  o.  At  the  moment  the  sharp  point  quitted  it  the  string  must  have  assumed 
the  position  of  fig.  i8  A  (p.  54a),  that  is,  it  must  have  formed  two  straight  lines 
proceeding  from  the  sharp  point  to  the  fixed  extremities  of  the  string.  Supposing 
the  position  of  the  sharp  point  at  that  moment  to  be  determined  by  a;  =  a  and 
y  =  6,  then  for  the  time  ^  =  o,  the  value  of  y^  was 

y=  —  if  a>a;>o (2) 

and  y  =  6.7^^if  Z>«>a  (2a) 

*— a 

and  the  values  of  y  in  (ic)  and  Y2),  or  else  (2a)  respectively,  must  be  identical. 
To  find  the  coefficient  A^^  tne  well-known  metiiod  is  to  multiply  both  sides  of 

the  equation  (ic)  by  sin  ^^ .  dx^  and  to  integrate  between  the  limits  0;= o  and 

x^L    In  this  case  equation  (ic)  reduces  to 

|V.sin!^.dx=^..|)in»'?p.«fe (2b) 

in  which  y  must  be  replaced  by  its  values  in  (2)  and  (2a).    Performing  the  into- 
grations  indicated  in  (2b)  we  find 

^"  =  mVa(Z-a)-"°-r  ^3> 


Digitized  by 


\^oogle 


376  MOTION  OF  PLUCKED  STRINGS.  app.  iii- 

Hence  A„  will  =s  o,  and  consequently  the  mOi  tone  of  the  string  will  disappear, 
when  sin  — ^-  =o,  that  is,  when  a  =  — or= -,  or  =  ^,  Ac.     Hence  if  we  sup- 

pose  the  string  to  be  divided  into  m  equal  parts,  and  to  be  plucked  in  one  of  these 
divisions,  the  mth  tone  disappears,  and  this  is  the  tone  whose  nodes  fail  upon 
these  points. 

Every  node  for  an  mth  tone  is  also  a  node  for  the  2n»th,  3mth,  4mth,  &c.,  tone, 
and  hence  all  these  tones  also  disappear. 

The  integral  of  equation  (i)  may  also,  as  is  well  known,  be  exhibited  in  the 

following  form : — 

y^<^(x^at)  +  ^x  +  at)   (4) 

T 
where  a'  =  — ,  and  ^,  \l/  are  arbitrary  functions.    The  function  <l>(X'-at)  denotes 

any  form  of  the  string  which  advances  in  the  direction  of  positive  x  with  the 
^  velocity  a,  but  without  any  other  change ;  and  the  fonotion  yl/(x  +  at)  denotes  a 
similar  form  proceeding  with  the  same  velocity  in  the  direction  of  negative  x. 
For  any  given  value  of  the  time  t  we  must  suppose  both  fonctions  to  be  given 
from  a;=— 00  tQa;=  +  oo,  and  then  the  motion  of  the  string  is  determined. 

The  determination  of  the  motion  of  a  plucked  string  will  result  in  this  second 
form  of  solution,  from  determining  the  functions  ^  and  \l^,  so  that 

1)  for  the  values  x  =  o  and  a;  =  2,  the  value  of  y  for  any  value  of  t  will  be 
constantly  ss  o.    This  will  be  the  case,  if  for  any  value  of  ^ 

^(  -a^)=  ^^+at) (4^) 

and  ^(Z  -  a^)  =  -  i/'(i  +  a^)   (4b) 

If  in  the  first  equation  we  put  at=  —v,  and  in  the  second  2  +  a^  =  —  t;,  we 
obtain 

iT  and  0(2Z  +  v)=  —  >/'(  —  '») 

so  that  ^(2i  +  v)  =  ^(v) (5) 

Hence  the  function  ^  is  periodic,  for  its  value  becomes  the  same  when  its 
argument  is  increased  by  2  2.    The  same  results  for  \j/. 

2)  For  t  =  o,  we  must  have  ^=  o  between  the  limits  a;  =  o  and  x^L    Henee 

at 

writing  }l/{v)  for  J^^^J,  and  putting  -^  =  o  in  equation  (4),  we  obtain 
av  at 

And  integrating  this  with  respect  to  x,  we  have 

^(a:)  =  i/^{x)  +  C 

^1       ISTow  since  neither  y  nor  -^  are  altered  by  adding  the  same  constant  to  f  and 

dt 

subtracting  it  from  4^,  the  constant  G  is  perfectly  arbitrary,  and  we  may  conse- 
quently assume  it  to  be  =  q,  and  hence  write 

9(^)^^x)    (5a) 

3)  Since  finally  at  the  time  t^o,  and  within  the  limits  x^o,  x^l^  the 
magnitude 

y,  which  is  =  ^(x)  +  i(x)  =  2<p{x), 

must  have  the  value  shewn  in  fig.  18  A  (p.  54a),  the  ordinates  of  this  figure  imme- 
diately give  the  value  of  2^(x)  and  of  2}l/(x),  by  means  of  equation  (5) : — 

between  x  =  o  and  x^zl 
„      x=2l    „  a;  =  3Z 
„      a;  =  4Z    „  x^s^ 
and  60  forth. 

Digitized  by  VjOOQIC 


^Fp.  m.  IV. 


SIMPLE  TONES  FEOM  BESONANCE. 


377 


But  since  from  (4a,  4b,  5)  it  follows  that  ^(  —  v)  =  —  ^(t?),  and  ([^(1  —  1?)  = 
—  f{l  +  v),  the  value  of  2^(0;)  is  given  by  the  triangle  in  fig.  18  G  (p.  54^)» 

between  a?  =—    iandaj=s     o 
„      a;=  —  3Z    „  a;  =  —  2Z 

and  in  the  same  way  between  x^        I    ,,  x^     2I 

and  so  forth. 

By  this  means  the  functions  ^  and  ij/  are  completely  determined,  and  on  sup- 
posing that  the  two  wave-Hnes  proceed  in  opposite  directions  with  the  velocity  a, 
we  obtain  the  forms  of  the  string  given  in  fig.  18,  p.  54a,  b,  which  represent  the 
changes  of  the  string  for  every  twelfth  part  of  the  periodic  time  of  its  vibration. 

[See  Donkin's  Acotistics,  Chaps.  V.  and  VI.] 


APPENDIX  IV. 

ON  THE   PRODUCTION   OF   SIBiPLE  TONES  BY  BESONANCE. 

(See  pp.  55a  and  69c.) 

I  HAVE  given  the  theory  of  tubes  and  hollow  spaces  filled  with  air,  so  fax  as  it  can 
be  at  present  mathematically  expressed,  in  my  paper,  entiiled  *  The  Theory  of 
Aerial  Vibrations  in  Tubes  with  Open  Ends*  (±heorie  der Luftschwingungen  in 
Bohren  mit  offenen  Etiden),  in  Crelle*s  Journal  filr  Mathematik,  vol.  Ivii.  A 
comparison  of  the  upper  partial  tones  of  tuning-forks  and  the  corresponding  reso- 
nance tubes,  will  be  found  in  my  paper,  *  On  Combinational  Tones '  ( Ueber  Com- 
hinationstone),  in  PoggendorflTs  Annalen^  vol.  xcix.  pp.  509  and  510.* 

I  add  here  the  dimensions  of  the  resonance  tubes  mentioned  on  p.  54a,  which 
were  made  for  me  by  Herr  Fessel,  in  Cologne,  in  connection  with  the  tuning-forks  m 
kept  in  motion  by  electricity  as  described  in  Appendix  VIII.  These  were  cylindrical 
tubes  of  pasteboard,  with  terminal  surfaces  of  zinc  plate,  one  entirely  closed,  the 
other  provided  with  a  circular  opening.  These  tubes  therefore  had  only  one 
opening,  not  two  like  the  resonators  which  were  intended  for  insertion  in  the  ear. 
A  resonance  tube  of  this  kind  can  have  its  tone  flattened  by  diminishing  its  opening. 
To  sharpen  the  tone,  when  necessary,  I  threw  in  a  little  wax,  and  placed  the  closed 
end  of  the  tube  on  a  warm  stove  or  hob,  until  the  wax  was  melted,  and  imifbrmly 
distributed  over  the  surface.  It  was  then  allowed  to  cool  in  the  same  position. 
To  try  whether  the  tone  of  a  tube  is  a  httle  sharper  or  flatter  than  that  of  the 
fork,  cover  its  opening  sHghtly  while  the  excited  fork  is  held  before  it.  If  the 
covering  strengthens  the  resonance  the  tube  was  too  sharp.  But  if  the  resonance 
begins  to  decrease  decidedly  as  soon  as  any  part  of  the  opening  is  covered,  the 
tube  was  too  flat.     The  dimensions  in  millimetres  [and  inches]  are  as  follows  : — 


No.  Pitch 

Length  of  Tube 

Diameter  of  Tube 

Diameter  of  Opening 

I    Bb 

425     [1673] 

138       [543] 
82       [323] 

315     [1-24] 

2    bb 

210       [827] 

23-5      [-93] 

3    /' 

117       [4-^iJ 

65       2-56J 

16           63] 

4    h'b 

88      [3-46] 

55       2-17] 

14-3       -56] 

5    d- 

58      [2-28] 

55       217] 

14          -55. 

6  r 

53      [2-09] 

44       173] 

12-5       -49 

7    a"b' 

50      [I-97J 

39       1-54] 

"•2       -44 

8    V'b 

40      [1-57] 

39      i:i-54] 

"•5       -45 

9    d"' 

35      [138] 

30-5    [I-20] 

10-3        41 

10   /'" 

26      [I -02] 

26      [I -02] 

8-5      [-34] 

The  theory  of  the  sympathetic  resonance  of  strings  is  best  developed  by  means 


*  The  hannonic  uppQr  partials  of  the  air 
▼ibrating  in  the  neighbourhood  of  a  tuning- 
fork,  there  mentioned,  have  also  been  observed 
with  an  interference  apparatus  by  Herr  Stefan 


{Proceedings  of  the  Vienna  Academy,  vol.  Izi. 
part  2,  pp.  491-8)  and  by  Herr  Quincke 
(Poggcndorfif's  Annals,  vol.  xxviii.). 


Digitized  by 


\^oogle 


378  SIMPLE  TONES  FEOM  RESONANCE.  app.  iv. 

of  the  experiments  mentioned  on  p.  55c.  Retain  the  notations  of  Appendix  III. 
and  assume  that  the  end  of  the  string  for  which  a;  =  o,  is  connected  with  the  stem 
of  the  tuning-fork,  and  must  move  in  the  same  way,  and  that  its  motion  is  given 
hy  the  equation 

y^^A  .  sinm^,    for  ic  =  o (6) 

Suppose  the  other  end  of  the  string  to  rest  on  the  bridge  which  stands  on  the 
sounding  board.     The  following  forces  act  upon  the  bridge : — 

i)  The  pressure  of  the  string,  which  will  increase  or  diminish  according  to  the 
angle  under  which  the  extremity  of  the  string  is  directed  against  the  bridge.  The 
tangent  of  this  angle  between  the  variable  direction  of  the  string  and  its  position 

of  rest  is  ^,  and  hence  we  can  put  the  variable  pressure  of  the  string  =  —  T.  -J', 

for  a;  =  Z,  supposing  the  bridge  to  lie  on  the  side  of  negative  y, 

2)  The  elastic  force  of  the  sounding  board,  which  acts  to  bring  the  bridge  back 
IT  into  its  position  of  rest,  may  be  put  =  — /V* 

3 )  The  sounding  board,  which  moves  with  the  bridge,  is  resisted  by  the  air,  to 
whicn  it  imparts  some  of  its  motion.  The  resistance  of  the  air  may  be  considered 
to  be  approximatively  proportional  to  the  velocity  of  its  motion,  and  hence  be 

Then  putting  M  for  the  mass  of  the  bridge,  we  obtain  the  following  equation 
for  the  motion  of  the  bridge,  and  hence  for  that  of  the  extremity  of  the  string 
which  rests  upon  it : 

^•S=-^-l-^^-^'4?'  ^'-' (^*) 

For  the  motion  of  the  other  points  in  the  string,  we  have,  as  in  Appendix  III., 
the  condition 

'•§=^-g <•> 

Since  part  of  every  motion  of  the  string  must  be  constantly  given  off  to  the 
air  in  the  resonance  chamber,  the  motion  would  gradually  die  away  if  it  were  not 
kept  up  by  some  continuous  cause.  Hence  we  may  neglect  the  variable  initial 
conditions  of  the  motion,  and  proceed  at  once  to  determine  the  periodic  motion, 
which  finally  remains  constant  under  the  influence  of  the  periodic  agitation  of 
the  tuning-fork.  It  is  manifest  that  the  period  of  the  motion  of  the  string  must 
be  the  same  as  the  period  of  the  motion  of  the  fork.  Hence  the  required  integral 
of  (i)  must  be  of  the  form 

y  =  Z>.  cosjpaj.  sin  m^  +  jB7.  cos  ^2;.  cos  mt\  /  x 

-{-F ,  sin^aj .  sin  mt-^-  G .  sin^a;.  cos  mtj 

And  to  satisfy  equation  (i)  we  must  then  have 

f  ^Trf^Tf (7a) 

From  the  equation  (7)  we  have,  when  a;  =30, 

y  =  D .  sin  mi  +  jE? .  cos  mi, 

and  on  comparing  this  with  equation  (6)  we  find 

I>  =  i4,  and  £  =  0 (8) 

The  two  other  coefficients  of  the  equation  (7),  namely  F  and  G,  must  be  deter- 
mined by  means  of  equation  (6a).  On  substituting  in  (6a)  the  values  of  y  from 
(7),  the  equation  (6a)  splits  into  two,  as  we  must  put  the  sum  of  the  terms  mul- 
tiplied by  sin  mi  separately  =  o,  and  also  the  sum  of  those  multiplied  by  cos  vni 
separately  =  o.    These  two  equations  are : 

F .  \{p-Mm^).  sin^^+2)!r.  cos ^q - Gmgr* .  sin |?r 

=—-4  .  [(/*— Jtfm') .  cos2?^-p!r.  sin^pZ]  /g^) 

-P77i^«.sinj5Z+a.[(/»-ilfm2).sinpi+2)r.cosi?q   ^     ' 

=— .4f/^wi.  cos  pZ 

^  Digitized  by  ^vjOOgie 


App.  rv. 


SIMPLE  TONES  FROM  RESONANCE. 


379 


Assume  for  abbreviation 

f*-Mm> 
(/»-Jifm«)«+l>»T«=C« 

Then  the  values  of  F  and  O  -will  be  as  follows : 


p__il    C* .  sin  2  (p?  +  ft)  +  g*m* .  sin  2pt 
1'  (P.  axx^  {pl  +  k)  +  g*m* .  sin^yi 


G  =  -A 


Cmg* .  sin  k 


(8b) 


(8c) 


C* .  sin"  (i?^  +  A;)  +  g*m^ .  sin'  jji 

Putting  the  amplitude  of  the  vibration  of  the  extremity  of  the  string  which 
rests  upon  the  bridge  =  F,  equation  (7)  becomes 

V^^[F.Bmpl  + A,  GOB pJ]^  +  CP.  sin« pi,  < 

and  on  putting  in  this  equation  the  values  of  F  and  G  from  (8c)  we  find 

AC .  sin  k 


F  = 


V  [0* .Bia^(pL  +  lc)+  g^w? .  sin  ^pj;\ 


(9) 


The  numerator  in  this  expression  is  independent  of  the  length  of  the  string. 
Any  alteration  of  its  length  therefore  affects  the  denominator  only.  Under  the 
radical  sign  is  the  sum  of  two  squares,  which  can  never  =  o,  because  m,  g,  p,  T, 
and  hence  k,  can  never  =  o.  The  coefficient  of  the  resistance  of  air,  g,  must  cer- 
tainly be  considered  as  infinitesimal.  Hence  the  denominator  is  a  maximum,  and 
F  is  a  minimum,  when  sin  (pi  +  A;)  =  o,  or  when 


pl  =  vTr—k , , 

where  v  is  any  whole  number.     The  maximum  value  of  F  is 

AC 


(9a) 


f 


F^  =  - 


g'm 


Hence,  other  circumstances  beiag  the  same,  this  maximum  value  increases  as 
Qy  the  coefficient  of  the  resistance  of  the  air,  decreases,  and  as  C  increases.  To 
ascertain  the  circumstances  on  which  the  magnitude  of  C  depends,  put  for  p^  in 
the  second  of  the  equations  (8b),  which  defines  the  meaning  of  C,  its  value  from 

(7a),  and  also  put  n*  =^r>;  this  gives 

C«  =  Jlf » .  (n«-m8)»  +  TfAmK 

Now  n  is  the  number  of  vibrations  which  the  bridge  would  perform  in  2t 
seconds,  under  the  influence  of  the  elastic  sounding  board  alone,  without  the 
string  and  the  resistance  of  the  air ;  and  m  is  the  same  number  of  vibrations  for 
the  tuning-fork.    Hence  the  maximum  value  of  F  can  now  be  written  f 


^'-V["'-(-5)'*^'-] 


in  which  everything  is  reduced  to  the  weights  M,  T,  fi  and  the  magnitude  of  the 

interval  i  —  J?  • 
m 

JSm>n,  which  is  usually  the  case,  it  is  advantageous  to  make  the  weight  of 
the  bridge  if,  rather  large.  Hence  I  have  had  it  constructed  of  a  plate  of  copper. 
If  Jlf  is  very  large,  k  will  be  very  small  in  consequence  of  (8b),  and  then  the  equa- 
tion (9a)  shews  that  the  varidus  tones  of  greatest  resonance  approach  all  the  more 
nearly  to  those  which  correspond  with  the  series  of  simple  whole  numbers.  The 
heavier  the  bridge  the  sharper  the  boundaries  of  the  tones  of  the  string. 

Observe  that  the  rules  here  given  for  the  influence  of  the  bridge  hold  only 
for  the  assumption  that  the  string  is  excited  by  a  tuning-fork,  and  not,  so  far  as 
this  investigation  extends,  for  other  cases. 

Digitized  by  VjOOQIC 


38o  VIBBATION  OF  PIANOFORTE  STRINGS.  app.  ▼. 

APPENDIX  V. 

ON   THB  VIBRATIONAIi   FOBMS  OF  PIANOFORTE   STRINGS. 

(See  pp.  74c  to  Sob.) 

When  a  stretched  string  is  struck  by  a  perfectly  hard  and  narrow  metal  point, 
which  is  immediately  withdrawn,  the  blow  conveys  a  certain  velocity  to  the  point 
struck,  while  the  rest  of  the  string  receives  no  motion.  Let  the  moment  of  im- 
pact correspond  to  ^  =  o  ;  the  motion  of  the  string  can  then  be  determined  on  the 
condition  that  at  that  moment  the  string  as  a  whole  was  in  its  position  of  equilibrium, 
and  that  it  was  only  the  point  struck  that  had  any  velocity.     Hence  in  equations 

-.  (ic)  and  (id)  of  Appendix  III.  (p.  3756)  put  both  y  =  o  and  -^^  =  o  for  ^  =  o,  at  all 

n  (It 

points  except  that  which  is  struck,  for  which  suppose  the  co-ordinate  to  be  a. 
Hence  it  follows  that  in  those  equations 

o  =  i4i  =  ila  =  il3  =  &c., 

and  the  values  of  E  are  determined  by  an  integration  similar  to  that  in  (2b), 
P-  375^1  giving 

J)  I  jodt  I 


and  vnmlB^  =  c .  sin 


Ttiva 


where  c  is  the  product  of  the  velocity  imparted  to  the  struck  portion  of  the  string 
and  of  its  infinitesimal  length.    Consequently 


vnl '  \ 


sm  y- .  sm  -y- .  Bin  2init 


+  -.  sin?I-  *  sin  -  .  sin  4-^nt  +  i.  sin  ^.  sin  5-^.  sin  6wnt  +  &c^ 

^  «-=ss-"°? <"" 

The  mth  partial  tone  of  the  string,  therefore,  disappears  in  this  case  also  when 
it  is  struck  in  a  node  of  this  string.  Also  the  upper  partial  tones  are  stronger  in 
comparison  with  the  prime  tone,  than  when  the  string  is  plucked,  because  the 
value  of  A^  in  equation  (3),  p.  %i^d,  has  m^  as  a  divisor,  whereas  the  value  of  B^  io 
(10)  has  only  m  as  a  divisor.  This  is  immediately  confirmed  by  experiment,  on 
striking  the  strings  with  the  sharp  edge  of  a  metal  ruler. 

For  a  pianoforte,  the  discontinuity  in  the  motion  of  the  string  is  diminished  by 
covering  the  hammer  with  an  elastic  pad.  This  sensibly  diminishes  the  force  df 
f  the  higher  upper  partials,  because  the  motion  is  no  longer  conveyed  to  a  single 
point,  but  is  imparted  to  a  sensible  length  of  string,  and  this  too,  not  in  an  indivi- 
sible  moment  of  time,  as  would  be  the  case  for  a  blow  with  a  perfectly  hard  body. 
On  the  contrary,  the  elastic  pad  yields  to  the  blow  at  first,  and  then  recovers  itself, 
so  that  while  the  hammer  is  in  contact  with  the  string,  the  motion  is  capable 
of  extending  over  a  considerable  length.  An  exact  ansJysis  of  the  motion  of  a 
string  excited  by  the  hammer  of  a  pianoforte  would  be  rather  compUcated.  But 
observing  that  Uie  string  moves  but  very  slightly  from  its  position  of  rest,  and 
that  the  elastic  pad  of  the  hammer  is  very  yielding  and  admits  of  much  com- 
pression, we  may  simplify  the  mathematical  theory,  by  assuming  the  pressure 
exerted  by  the  hammer  during  the  blow  which  it  gives  to  the  string  to  be  as  great 
as  it  would  be  if  the  string  were  a  perfectly  fixed  and  perfectly  unyielding  body. 
AVe  are  then  able  to  assume  the  pressure  of  the  hammer  to  be 

P  =  i4  sin  mU 

for  such  moments  of  time  that  o<^<-.    This  last  magnitude  ^  is  the  length  of 

771  m 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQiC 


APP.v.  VIBRATION  OF  PIANOFORTE   STRINGS.  381 

time  during  which  the  hammer  iff  in  contact  with  the  string.  The  magnitude  of 
m  increases  therefore  as  the  elastic  power  of  the  hammer  increases  and  its  weight 
decreases. 

We  have  first  to  determine  the  motion  of  the  string  during  the  interval  of  time 
that  the  hanmier  is  in  contact  with  it,  that  is,  from  ^  =  0  to  ^=  ~.    During  this 

time,  the  hammer  divides  the  string  into  two  sections,  and  the  motion  of  each 
section  has  to  be  separately  determined.  At  the  place  of  impact  let  x  be  written 
Xq.  When  xKx^,  distinguish  the  values  of  y  by  writing  them  y,,  and  when  x>Xq, 
by  writing  them  y^.  At  the  point  struck  the  pressure  of  the  string  against  the 
hammer  must  be  equal  to  the  pressure  P,  which  the  hammer  exerts  against  the 
string.  The  pressure  of  the  string  is  to  be  calculated  as  in  Appendix  IV.,  equation 
(6a)  (p.  3786),  and  we  consequenUy  obtain  the  equation 

P=^.dn^=r. (*-!') (..,    t 

Waves  proceed  towards  both  ends  of  the  string  from  the  place  struck.    Hence 
yi  must  have  the  form 

for  values  of  tj  for  which  o  <  ^  <  ^,  and  fl;o>  x  >  Xq—  at,  and  y^  must  have  the 

m 

form 

for  the  same  values  of  ^  and  for  values  of  x  for  which  tCo  <  ^  <  ^0  +  ^^«  Using  f 
for  the  differential  coefficient  of  the  function  ^,  equation  (11)  gives 

P=:A.saimt=^2T.<i^'  (at) (iia) 

Integrating  with  respect  to  ^  we  find  % 

C-  — .  cosmi=-^.  A  (at), 
m  a 

and  then,  determining  the  constant  C,  so  that  yi  =  o  when  js  =  Xo  +  at,  and  y ^  =  o 
when  x=^Xq  —  at,  we  have 


2mT 


<  I  —  cos  I -(a;  —Xo)  +  mt\  \ , 
I  i-cosf— (aJo-«)  +  mM  j. 


This  determines  the  motion  of  the  string  for  the  time  t,  when  o  <  ^  <  _ ,  and 

m 

on  the  supposition  that  the  two  waves  proceeding  firom  the  place  of  impact  have 

not  reached  one  of  the  ends  of  the  string.    If  the  latter  had  been  the  case,  tibey  ^ 

would  have  been  reflected  there. 

When  at  has  become  greater  than  ^ ,  the  pressure  P  will  be  ss  o,  and  hence  it 

follows  from  equation  (iia)  that  from  thenceforward 

<&'  (at)^o,  and  hence  ^=s  constant,  when  at  >^. 

m 

aA 
Hence  both  y^  and  y^  remain  =  — =  for  all  those  parts  of  the  string  over  which 

the  waves  have  already  advanced,  until  portions  of  the  waves  reflected  from  the 
extremities  reach  those  parts  of  the  string  on  their  return. 

To  introduce  the  influence  of  the  extremities  of  the  string  properly  into  calcu- 
lation, suppose  the  string  to  be  infinitely  long,  and  that  at  all  points  distant  from 
Xq  by  multiples  of  2I,  similar  blows  are  given  to  it,  so  that  from  all  these  places 


Digitized  by  VjOOQ IC 


382  VIBRATION   OF  PIANOFORTE   STRINGS.  app.  v. 

waves  proceed  similar  to  those  which  proceed  from  Xq.  Moreover  suppose  that  in 
all  those  places  for  which  x  =  —  Xq^  2yl,  a  blow  be  applied  equal  to  that  given  to 
Xq  and  at  the  same  time,  but  in  the  opposite  direction,  so  that  from  all  these  latter 
points  waves  will  proceed  of  an  identical  form,  but  with  a  negative  height.  Those 
points  of  the  infinite  string  which  correspond  with  the  extremities  of  the  finite 
string  will  then  be  agitated  by  positive  and  negative  waves  of  equal  magnitude  at 
the  same  time,  and  will  hence  be  completely  at  rest,  and  consequently  all  the 
conditions  of  the  real  finite  string  will  be  fulfilled  by  the  state  of  this  section  of 
the  infinite  string. 

From  the  moment  that  the  hammer  quits  the  string,  the  motion  of  the  string 
may  be  regarded  as  two  systems  of  waves,  one  advancing  (or  in  the  direction  of 
positive  x),  and  the  other  retreating  (or  in  the  direction  of  negative  x).  Of  these 
systems  of  waves  we  have  as  yet  found  only  certain  isolated  portions,  namely 
those  which  correspond  to  the  sections  of  the  string  lying  nearest  the  point  struck. 
We  have  now  to  complete  these  waves  properly  and  obtain  a  connected  advancing 
IF  and  retreating  system. 

Advancing  in  the  direction  of  the  positive  x  on  the  string,  we  have  y  =  o  untfl 

we  come  to  a  positive  retreating  wave,  and  then  it  rises  to  ~-^,  which  is  its  value 

in  the  positive  striking  points.  If  we  proceed  beyond  the  striking  point,  and  over 
the  wave  thence  proceeding,  we  again  find  values  of  y  which  =  o,  and  sink  to 

——-  as  soon  as  the  first  negative  retreating  wave  has  been  passed  over.     This  is 

the  value  of  y  in  the  first  negative  striking  point.  To  connect  the  positive  and 
negative  retreating  waves  properly  with  each  other,  we  must  suppose  the  values  of 
yi  to  be  increased  between  every  positive  striking  point  and  the  next  foUowing 

negative  striking  point,  by  the  magnitude  +  ^,  so  that  the  height  of  the  wave 

retains  this  value,  which  it  had  at  Xq^  until  the  corresponding  negative  wave  begins. 

■r  Here  then  the  height  of  the  wave  becomes  —  rn'^Vi  ^^^  sinks  to  zero.   Similarly, 

suppose  that =is  added  to  the  height  of  the  wave  in  advancing  waves  between 

ml 

any  negative  striking  point  and  the  next  following  positive  striking  point.  The 
retreating  waves  will  thus  be  everywhere  positive,  and  the  advancing  waves  every- 
where negative,  and  the  waves  at  the  same  time  are  so  constituted  that  their  con- 
tinued motion  will  generate  that  kind  of  motion  which  we  have  found  to  exist  in 
the  string  after  the  hammer  quits  it. 

We  have  now  to  express  this  system  of  waves  as  the  sum  of  simple  waves. 
The  length  of  the  wave  is  2Z,  because  the  points  of  simultaneous  impact  lie  at 

intervals  of  2h    Let  us  take  the  positive  retreating  waves  at  the  time  tss- 

then 

i)y,  =0,  from  a?  =  o  to  a?  =  a:o— — 


from  a;  =  a?o— —   to  x^x^ 
m 

3)2/1=— 7Ti»    from    X  =  Xo    to    iC=2/— Xo— — 

ml  VI 

from  a;  =  2Z-rro-—  to  ar  =  2Z-a-o 
m 

5)  ^1  =  o»  from  X  =  2^—3^0  to  rr  =  2/. 


Digitized  by 


Google 


App.  V.  VIBRATION  OF  PIANOFORTE   STRINGS.  383 

Hence  if  we  assume 

y ,  =  ilo  +  ill .  cos  J  (a;  +  c)  +  ^a .  cos  ^J  (a?  +  c)  +  ilg .  cos  ^(x  +  c)  -\-  &c. 

+  Bi .  siny  (x  +  c)+B^. sin  y  (a;  +  c)  +  J?, .  sin  35  (x -^  c) -{-  &o.  (12) 


we  shall  have 


*'  .nw, 


I  yi .  cos  ^  («  +  c) .  da;  =  A  J, 
yi .  sin  !^  (a?  +  c) .  da;  =  JB„Z. 


^ 


If  we  put  c  =  — ,  every  B  becomes  =  o,  because  yi  has  the  same  values  for 

—  +  (  and  —  —  (,  and  the  limits  of  the  integration  may  be  selected  at  pleasure  «r 
provided  only  that  their  difference  is  2Z.    But  on  the  other  hand 

1                    2aA7nP               '^  fnv    _  N    ^^^  fnv   av\  ,      . 

K^—in r-n-a am-  sm  (  ^  .a;© ) .  cos  ( -^  .  ^  )  (12a) 

This  equation  gives  the  amplitude  A^  of  the  several  partial  tones  of  the  com- 
pound tone  of  the  string  which  has  been  struck.    When  the  point  of  impact  is  a 

node  of  the  nth  partial,  the  factor  sin  [  ^ .  x^  will  =  o,  and  hence  all  those  par- 
tial tones  disappear  which  have  a  node  at  the  point  of  impact.  The  table  on 
p.  79c  was  calculated  from  this  equation.* 

To  determine  the  complete  motion  of  the  string  we  must  further  substitute 
a;  -(-  a^  for  a;  in  the  equation  (12)  for  y^  The  corresponding  expression  for  y^  then 
becomes 

y*=— ilo""-^! -coSy  (a;  +  ai—c)— -4s. cos  y  (a;— a^— c)— Ac.  ^ 

and  finally 
y  =  yi  +  y>  =  2A1 .  COS  J  a? .  COS  ^  {at  +c)  +  ^A^ .  cos  —  a; .  cos  ^  {at  +  c)  +  &c- 

which  completely  solves  the  problem. 

If  m  be  infinite,  that  is,  if  the  hammer  be  perfectly  hard,  the  expression  for 
A^  in  (12a)  becomes  identical  with  that  of  B^  in  equation  (10),  p.  380c.  It  must  be 
remembered  that  m  in  (10)  is  identical  with  n  in  (12a)  (and  that  a  in  (10)  is  then 
identical  with  Xq  in  (i2aj,  while  a  in  (12a)  has  a  different  meaning). 

If  m  is  not  infinite,  as  n  increases  the  coefficients  il«  decrease  as  ^,  but  if  m 

be  infinite  they  decrease  as  i- ;  for  plucked  strings  they  decrease  as  -^.  This  corrc' 

spends  to  the  theorems  proved  by  Stokes   {Cambridge  Transactions,  vol.  viii. 
PP-  533  to  5S4)  concerning  the  effect  of  the  discontinuity  of  a  function,  when  f 
developed  in  Fourier's  series,  upon  the  magnitude  of  the  terms  with  high  ordinal 
numbers.    Thus,  if  y  is  the  fdnction  to  be  developed  in  a  series 

y  =  ilo  +  -4i .  sin  {ma  +  Ci)  +  A^  .  sin  {2fnz  +  c^  +  &c. 

the  coefficient  of  A^  when  n  is  very  great, 

I )  is  of  the  order  L  when  y  itself  suddenly  alters ; 
n 

*  [It  is  shewn  in  the  notes  on  p.  76(2'  and  foroe  of  the  corresponding  partial  is  materially 
p.  77c,  that  when  the  blow  is  made  with  an  weakened,  it  is  not  absolutely  extinguished, 
ordinary  pianoforte  hammer,  the  partial  tone,  It  may  therefore  be  necessaiy  to  re-open  the 
eorresponding  to  the  node  struck,  does  not  mathematical  investigation  without  having  re- 
wholly  vanish.  The  subject  is  resumed  in  course  to  the  facilitation  due  to  the  funda- 
App.  XX.  sect.  N.  No.  2,  where  the  result  of  mental  (but  certainly  only  approximative)  as- 
later  experiments  is  given.  In  the  meantime  sumption  on  p.  'fiod,  giving  P  =  ^  sin  nU. — 
it  must  be  borne  in  mind  that  though  the  TranslcUor,"] 


Digitized  by 


\^oogle 


384  MOTION  OF  VIOLIN   STRINGS.  app.  v.  vi, 

2)  is  of  the  order  -^  when  the  first  differential  coefficient  S  suddenly  alters ; 

3)  is  of  the  order  J^  when  the  second  differential  coefficient  -^  suddenly 

alters ; 

4)  is  at  most  of  the  order  e-"  when  the  function  itself  and  all  its  differential 
coefficients  are  continuous.     [See  note,  p.  35^2.] 

Hence  follow  the  laws  of  musical  tones  so  often  mentioned  in  the  text,  that 
their  upper  partial  tones  generally  increase  in  power,  with  the  greater  discontinuity 
of  the  corresponding  motion  of  the  resonant  body. 

[See  Donldn's  Acoicstics,  pp.  1 19-126,  where,  on  p.  124,  equation  (14)  corre* 
^  spends  to  equation  (12a)  above.] 


APPENDIX  VI. 

ANALYSIS  OP  THE   MOTION  OP  VIOLIN  STRINGS. 

(See  p.  83a.) 

AssuBfE  the  lens  of  the  vibration  microscope  to  make  horizontal  vibrations,  then 
vibrational  curves  wiU  be  observed  like  those  represented  in  fig.  23,  p.  826,  c.  Call 
the  vertical  ordinate  y  and  the  horizontal  x ;  then  y  is  directly  proportional  to  Uie 
displacement  of  the  vibrating  point,  and  x  to  that  of  the  vibrating  lens.  Tlie 
latter  performs  a  simple  pendular  vibration.  If  the  number  of  its  vibrations  be  n 
^  and  the  time  t,  we  have  generally 

re  =s  4  .  sin  {iimt  +  c) 

where  A  and  c  are  constant. 

Now  if  y  also  makes  n  vibrations,  x  and  y  are  both  periodic  and  have  the 
same  periodic  time.  Hence,  at  the  end  of  each  period,  x  and  y  have  the  same 
values  as  before,  and  the  observed  point  is  at  exactly  the  same  place  as  at  the 
beginning  of  the  period.  This  holds  for  every  point  in  the  curve  and  for  every 
fresh  repetition  of  the  vibratory  motion,  so  that  the  curve  appears  stationary. 

Suppose  a  vibrational  form  of  the  kind  depicted  in  figs.  5,  6,  7,  8,  9,  pp.  20 
and  21,  in  which  the  horizontal  abscissa  is  directly  proportional  to  the  time,  to 
be  wrapped  round  a  cylinder^  of  which  the  circumference  is  equal  to  a  single 
period  of  those  curves,  so  that  the  time  t  is  now  to  be  measured  along  the  cir- 
cumference of  the  cylinder.  Gall  x  the  distance  of  a  point  from  a  plane  drawn 
through  the  axis  of  the  cylinder.    Then  in  this  case  also 

^  a;  =  -4  .  sin  (2imt  +  c), 

where  ii  .  sin  c  is  the  value  of  x  for  ^  =  o»  and  A  is  the  radius  of  the  cylinder. 
Hence,  if  the  curve  drawn  upon  the  cylinder  be  viewed  by  an  eye  at  an  infinite 
distance  in  the  line  x  =s  o,  ^  =  o,  the  curve  has  exactly  tide  same  appearance  as  in 
the  vibration  microscope. 

If  X  and  y  have  not  exactly  the  same  period ;  if,  for  example,  y  makes  n  vibra- 
tions while  X  makes  n  +  A  n,  where  A  n  is  a  very  small  number,  Uie  expression  for 
X  may  be  written 

a?  =  il  .  sin  [2imt  +  (c  +  2irtAn)]. 

In  this  case,  then,  c  which  was  formerly  constant,  increases  slowly.  But  c  r«> 
presents  the  angle  between  the  plane  x  =0  and  the  point  in  the  drawing  for  which 
^  =  o.  In  this  case,  then,  the  imaginary  cylinder  round  which  the  drawing  is 
supposed  to  be  wrapped,  revolves  slowly. 

Since  a  magnitude  which  is  periodic  after  the  period  ^,  may  be  also  considered 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


APP.  VI.  MOTION  OF  VIOLIN  STRINGS.  385 

as  periodic  after  the  periods  2ir,  or  3ir,  or  I'lr,  where  r  is  any  whole  number,  these 
remarks  apply  also  for  the  case  when  the  period  of  y  is  an  aliquot  part  of  the 
period  of  x,  or  conversely,  or  both  are  aliquot  parts  of  the  same  third  period,  that 
is,  for  the  case  when  the  tones  of  the  tuning-fork  and  of  the  observed  body  stand  in 
any  consonant  ratio  ;  the  only  limitation  is  that  the  common  period  of  vibration 
must  not  exceed  the  time  required  for  a  luminous  impression  to  become  extinct 
in  the  eye.     [See  Donkin's  Acoustics,  pp.  36-44.] 

Prom  the  observed  curves,  fig.  23  B,  C,  p.  826,  and  fig.  24  A,  B,  p.  836,  it 
follows  that  all  points  of  the  string  ascend  and  descend  alternately,  that  the  ascent 
is  made  with  a  constant  velocity,  and  also  the  descent  with  a  constant  velocity, 
which  is  however  different  from  the  velocity  of  ascent.  When  the  bow  is  drawn 
across  a  node  of  one  of  the  upper  partials  of  the  string,  the  motion  takes  place  in 
all  nodes  of  the  same  tone  precisely  in  the  manner  described.  For  other  points 
of  the  string,  little  crumples  are  perceptible  in  the  vibrational  figure,  but  they  do 
not  prevent  us  from  clearly  recognising  the  principal  motion,  IT 

Pia.  63. 


If  in  fig.  62  we  reckon  the  time  from  the  abscissa  of  the  point  a,  so  that  for 
a,  ^  =  o,  and  further  for  the  point  fi  put  ^  =  r,  and  for  the  point  y  put  ^  =  T,  so 
that  the  last  represents  the  length  of  a  whole  period ;  then  * 

yss/^  +  &,  from  ^  =  0  to  ^asr;              1  ,^ 

y  =zg  {T -t)-^h,  from  t=^T  to  t^T,  J    ^'' 

whence  for  ^  =  r,  it  results  that 

fr=g{T-T).  1 

Now  suppose  y  to  be  developed  in  one  of  Foiurier's  series  : 

y  =  il, .  sin  ?^  +  -4a .  sin  ^  +  ^8  •  sin  -^  &o. 

+  Bi.C08^+JB,.C0S^*  +  B,,C0S^&C. 

then  it  results  from  integration  that 

and  this  gives  the  following  values  for  A^  and  B^ : 

and  y  may  then  be  written  in  the  form 

In  equation  (2),  y  denotes  merely  the  distance  of  any  determinate  point  of  the 
string  from  its  position  of  rest.    U  x  denotes  the  distance  of  this  point  from  the 

Digitized  by  ^vJ (5 (Sgie 


386  MOTION  OF  VIOLIN   STRINGS.  app.  vt. 

beginning  of  the  string,  and  L  the  length  of  the  string,  then  the  general  form  of 
y,  as  in  equation  (ib)  of  App.  III.,  p.  5756,  is 

+2::r  {^-"-^-^('-i)} 13) 

By  comparing  equations  (2)  and  (3)  we  find  immediately  that  all  D*8  Yanish, 
or 

2)^  =  o,  and 

n  .sin^^=^.-^..8in5!^ (3a) 

^       Here  g  +/  and  r  are  independent  of  x^  but  not  of  n.    On  taking  the  equations 
for  n=  I  and  n^2,  and  then,  dividing  one  by  the  other,  there  residts 

^«.C08  2=-.008  5,. 

T  m 

From  which  it  follows  that  for  «  =  -,  t  is  also=  -,  as  observation  shews. 

2  2 

But  if  a;  =  o,  then  according  to  observations  r  is  also  =  o.     Hence 

C,  =  i.CH    andj^l,    (3b) 

so  that  ^  +/  i&  independent  of  x.    Let  v  be  the  amplitude  of  the  vibration  of  the 
point  X  in  the  string,  then 

IF  /r  =  5r(T-T)  =  2t;, 

„    .    ^       2V    ,      2V  2VT  2VL* 


T      T-T     r(T^T)     Txijj-x) 
And  since  ^  +/  is  independent  of  x,  we  must  have 

where  Fis  the  amplitude  in  the  middle  of  the  string.  From  equation  (3b)  it  follows 
that  the  sections  a/3  and  fiy  of  the  vibrational  figure,  fig.  62,  p.  3856,  must  be 
proportional  to  the  corresponding  parts  of  the  string  on  both  sides  of  the  observed 
point.    Hence  we  have  finally 

for  the  complete  expression  of  the  motion  of  the  string. 

If  we  put  t— -  =0,  y  will  s=o  for  all  values  of  x,  and  hence  all  parts  of  the 

string  pass  through  their  position  of  rest  simultaneously.    From  that  time  the 

velocity/  of  the  point  x  is 

,_2v_SV(L'-x) 

^^V LT 

TV 
But  this  velocity  lasts  only  during  the  time  r,  where  r=s  -  .    Hence  after  the 

time  t,  and 

Tx                              SV 
as  long  as  ^<  -r-»  ^®  ^*^®  y='f^^f-m-  (i  — «)^ (4) 


SV 
and  hence  y  <  ^=^,x(L''x), 


Digitized  by 


Google 


APP.  VI.  MOTION  OP  VIOLIN  STRINGS.  387 

From  that  point  y  retumB  with  the  velocity 

2V        8  7x 
9  —  i 


■-T      '   JjT 

And  hence  after  the  time  <s=r  4-  ti. 

And  since  L  —  a:  =  — >y,-  •  L 

we  find  y=^.{r-(r  +  «.)} 

=^.  (r-«) (4a)    f 

The  deflection  on  one  part  of  the  string  is  therefore  given  bv  the  equation  (4), 
and  on  the  other  part  by  the  equation  ^4a).  Both  equations  show  that  the  form 
of  the  string  is  a  straight  line,  which  m  (4)  passes  through  x^L,  and  in  (4a) 
through  a;  =  o.  These  are  the  two  extremities  of  the  string.  The  point  where 
these  straight  hnes  intersect  is  given  by  the  condition 

Whence  (L-x)t^x(T-i) 

and  Lt^xT. 

Hence  the  abscissa  x  of  the  point  of  intersection  increases  in  proportion  to  the 
time.    This  point  of  intersection,  which  is  at  the  same  time  the  point  of  the  string 
most  remote  from  the  position  of  rest,  passes,  therefore,  with  a  constant  velocity 
from  one  end  of  the  string  to  the  other,  and  during  this  passage  describes  a  ^ 
parabolic  arc,  for  which 

Hence  the  motion  of  the  string  may  be  briefly  thus  described.  In  fig.  63  the 
foot  d  of  the  ordinate  of  its  highest  point  moves  backwards  and  forwards  with  a 
constant  velocity  on  the  horizontal  line  ab, 

Fio.  63. 


while  the  highest  point  of  the  string  describes  in  succession  the  two  parabolic  arcs 
acib  and  bcsa,  and  the  string  itself  is  always  stretched  in  the  two  lines  aC|  and 
bo,  or  aca  and  bcj.     [See  Donkin's  Acoustics,  pp.  131-138.] 

The  small  crumples  of  the  vibrational  figures  which  are  so  frequently  observed, 
fif?'  25,  p.  846,  probably  arise  from  the  damping  and  disappearance  of  those  tones 
which  have  nodes  at  the  point  bowed  or  in  its  immediate  neighbourhood,  and 
are  consequently  either  unexcited  or  but  slightly  excited  by  the  bow.  When  the 
bow  is  drawn  across  the  string  in  a  node  of  the  mth  partial  tone  situate  near  to 
the  bridge,  the  vibrations  of  this  mth,  and  further  of  the  2mih,  3mth,  &c.,  tone 
have  no  influence  on  the  motion  of  the  point  in  the  string  touched  by  the  bow, 
and  they  may  consequently  disappear,  wi&out  changing  the  effect  of  the  bow  upon 
the  string,  and  this  really  explains  the  crumples  observed  in  the  vibrational  figure. 
[See  also  App.  XX.  sect.  N.  No.  5,  on  Prof.  Mayer's  Harmonic  Curves.]  I  have 
not  been  able  to  determine  by  observation  what  happens  when  the  bow  is  drawn 
across  the  string  between  two  nodal  points. 

Digitized  by  VjCft3^lC 


38S  THEORY  OF  PIPES.  app.  \^I. 

APPENDIX  Vn. 

ON   THE   THBOBY  OP  PIPES* 

A.  Influence  of  Resonance  in  Reed  Pipes. 
(See  p.  1026.) 

•  The  laws  of  resonance  for  cylindrical  tubes  have  been  developed  maihematicallj 
in  my  paper  on  the  *  Theory  of  Aerial  Vibrations  in  Tubes  with  Open  Ends ' 
(Theorie  der  Luftschwingungen  in  Rdhren  mit  offenen  Enden,  *  Journal  fiir  reine 
und  angewandte  Mathematik/  vol.  Ivii.).  The  example  treated  in  §  7  of  that  paper 
is  applicable  to  reed  pipes,  where  the  motion  at  the  bottom  of  the  pipe  is  assumed 

%  to  be  given.  Let  Vdt  be  the  volume  of  air  which  enters  the  reed  pipe  in  the  in- 
finitesimal time  dtj  then  as  this  magnitude  is  periodical  we  can  express  Fin  one  of 
Fourier's  series,  thus  : — 

F=  Co  +  Ci .  cos  {2irnt  +  Tx)  +  Cj .  cos  (41m*  +  ^2)  +  &c (i) 

The  resonance  must  be  determined  separately  for  each  term,  because  the  vibra- 
tions corresponding  to  each  partial  tone  are  superposed  without  modification. 
If  we  assume  Z  to  be  4ihe  length  of  the  tube,  S  its  section,  Z  +  a  the  reduced 
length  of  the  tube  (where  in  cylindrical  tubes  the  difference  a  is  equal  to  the 

radius  multiplied  by  ^  [but  see  p.  9ieZ,  note  t]),  fe  the  magnitude  ^  (where  X  is  the 

4  ^ 

length  of  the  wave),  and  put  the  potential  of  the  wave  in  free  space,  for  the  tone 
having  the  vibrational  number  i^n. 


% 


=  .J! ,  cos  [vkr^  irmt  +  c), 


where  r  is  the  distance  from  the  middle  of  the  opening;  then  the  equations  (15) 
and  (12b)  of  the  paper  referred  to,  give 


V  (4fl-«  cos«  rk  (Z  r  a)  4-  y*k*S^  sin^  vkl) 


Since  the  magnitude  k^S  must  always  be  considered  as  very  small  to  make  our 
theory  applicable,  this  equation,  for  cases  in  which  Z  +  a  is  not  an  uneven  mul- 
tiple of  the  length  of  a  quarter  of  a  wave,  becomes  approximatively, 

3f^— 9i . 

2T.C0Sr^(Z  +  a)* 

Hence  the  resonance  is  weakest  when  the  reduced  length  of  the  tube  is  an 
even  multiple  of  the  length  of  a  quarter  of  a  wave,  and  becomes  stronger  as  it 
>[  approaches  an  uneven  multiple  of  that  length.    When  it  absolutely  reaches  such 
a  multiple  the  complete  formula  gives 

Hence  the  maximum  of  resonance  increases  as  the  length  of  the  wave  of  the 
tone  in  question  increases  and  the  transverse  section  decreases.  The  smaller  the 
transverse  section,  the  more  sharply  defined  is  the  limit  of  the  pitch  of  the  tone 
which  is  strongly  reinforced  by  resonance  ;  while  when  the  transverse  section  is 
large,  the  reinforcement  of  resonance  extends  over  a  much  greater  length  of  the 
scale. 

For  hollow  bodies  of  other  shapes,  with  naiTow  mouths,  similar  equations 
may  be  obtained  by  means  of  the  propositions  given  in  §  10  of  the  paper  cited. 

Since  the  condition  of  powerful  resonance  is  that  cos  lA;  (Z  -H  o)  =  o,  cylin- 
drical tubes  (clarinet)  reinforce  only  the  prime  and  other  unevenly  numbered  partial 
tones  [but  see  note  p.  99c]. 

Digitized  by  V^OOQIC 


APP.  vn.  THEORY  OF  PIPES.  389 

In  the  interior  of  conical  tubes  we  may  assume  the  potential  of  the  motion  of 
the  air  for  the  tube  n  to  be 

F=  -  .  sin  (ftr  +  c)  cos  2nnL 

T 

where  r  is  the  distance  from  the  vertex  of  the  cone.  If  a  vibrator  is  introduced  at 
a  distance  a  from  the  vertex,  and  I  be  the  length  of  the  tube,  so  that  for  the  open 
end  r  =  Z  +  a,  we  may  assume  as  an  approximatively  correct  Umiting  condition  for 
the  free  end,  that  the  pressure  there  vanishes.    This  is  the  case  when 

dV  A 

-3-  =  — 2?m . . .  sin  [k{l  4-  a)  +  c] .  sin  2nnt  =  o,    and  hence 

at  I  +  a 


Hence  we  may  assume 


sin  [k{l  +  a)  +  c]  =  ©• 


A 

and  F=  -  ,  sin  fc(r— i— a) .  cos  2imL 

T 

The  most  powerful  resonance,  then,  in  this  case,  as  well  as  in  cylindrical  tubes, 
belongs  to  those  tones  which  have  a  minimum  velocity  at  the  place  where  the 
vibrator  is  placed.  For  as  during  the  development  of  velocity  in  the  mouthpiece  in 
equation  (i)  the  coefficients  C^  have  a  determinate  value  which  depends  only  on 
the  motion  of  the  vibrator  and  the  pulses  of  air  which  it  occasions,  the  coefficient 
A  of  the  last  equation  must  increase,  as  the  velocity  produced  by  the  correspond- 
ing train  of  waves  in  the  mouthpiece  of  the  tube  decreases.  The  velocity  in  the 
other  parts  of  the  tube  will  then  increase.    Now  the  velocity  of  a  particle  of  air  is 

dV     A 

=  _- .  cos  2rmt  •  [kr  •  cos  fc(r— i— a)— sin  fe(r— i— a)]. 

Hence  for  maximum  resonance  we  have  the  condition  that  for  r  s  a  ^ 

either  hr  =  tan  A;(r— Z— a) 

or       ia  =  — tanfcZ. 

If,  then,  the  magnitude  a,  that  is,  the  distance  of  the  vibrator  from  the  vertex 
of  the  cone,  be  very  small,  ka  and  also  tan  kl  will  be  very  small,  and  kl—w  must 
also  be  very  small,  if  i'  is  any  whole  number.  Hence  we  may  develop  the  tangents 
according  to  powers  of  their  arc,  and  retaining  only  the  first  term  of  this  develop- 
ment we  have 

ka  =  mr—  kl,    and    k{a  +  Z)  =  ^t 

or,  putting    fc  =  ~ ,    we  have 

A 

a  +  l=iy.-. 

f 

This  shews  that  conical  tubes  reinforce  all  ihose  tones  for  which  the  whole  length 
of  the  cone,  reckoned  up  to  its  imaginary  vertex,  is  a  multiple  of  half  the  length 
of  the  wave,  on  the  assumption  that  the  distance  of  the  vibrator  from  this  ima- 
ginary vertex  is  infinitesimal  in  comparison  with  the  length  of  the  wave»  Hence 
if  the  prime  of  the  compound  tone  is  reinforced  by  the  tube,  all  the  partial  tones, 
both  the  evenly  and  unevenly  numbered^  will  be  reinforced  up  to  a  pitch  where 
the  wave-lengths  of  the  higher  partial  tones  cease  to  be  very  large  in  comparison 
with  the  distance  a.* 

CcBteris  paribus,  the  number  and  magnitude  of  the  terms  of  the  series  (i), 
which  represents  the  exciting  aerial  motion  will  be  the  greater,  the  more  perfectly 
the  entering  stream  of  air  is  interrupted.    Free  reeds  must  therefore  tit  their 

*  [The  remainder  of  this  Appendix  VII.  to      principally  from  the   ist  English  edition.-— 
p.  3966,  is  an  addition  to  the  4th  German      Translator.] 
edition.     The  additions    on    pp.    396-7    are 

Digitized  by  VjOOQIC 


390  THEOBY  OF  PIPES.  app.  vii. 

frames  very  exactly,  in  order  to  produce  a  powerful  tone.  Strikmg  reeds,  which 
effect  a  more  perfect  stoppage,  are  superior  in  this  respect.  According  to  the 
information  obtained  by  Mr.  A.  J.  Ellis  [see  p.  95^^',  note  t],  organ-builders  have 
really  been  more  inclined  in  recent  times  to  use  striking  reeds.  But  the  vibrating 
laminsB  are  very  sUghtly  curved,  so  that  they  do  not  strike  the  frame  all  at  once, 
but  roll  themselves  gradually  out  upon  it. 

B.  Theory  of  the  Blotuing  of  Pipes. 

When  longitudinal  waves  have  once  been  excited  in  the  mass  of  air  in  a  tube, 
they  may  be  reflected  backwards  and  forwards  many  times  between  the  ends  of 
the  tube,  and  form  constant,  periodically  returning  vibrations,  before  they  die 
away.  At  the  closed  end  of  a  stopped  pipe,  the  reflexion  of  ever^  train  of  waves 
is  tolerably  complete,  but  at  the  open  ends  a  perceptible  fraction  of  the  wave 
always  passes  into  the  open  air,  and  hence  the  reflected  wave  has  not  the  same 
f  intensity  as  the  incident  wave  possessed.  Indeed  the  intensity  of  the  waves  re- 
flected backwards  and  forwards  in  the  tube  continually  diminishes,  and  finally 
dies  off,  if  the  lost  work  is  not  replaced  at  every  backwards  and  forwards  reflexion 
by  some  other  kind  of  action.  What  has  to  be  replaced,  however,  is  usually  only 
a  small  part  of  the  whole  vis  viva  of  the  modulatory  motion,  that  is,  just  as  much 
as  is  lost  by  reflexion  at  the  open  ends.  If  the  inner  radius  at  the  open  end  of  a 
cylindrical  tube  be  i?,  the  fraction  of  the  amplitude  which  is  lost  at  the  open  end 
for  a  tone  having  the  wave-length  A,  is,  according  to  theory, 

where  B  is  small  in  comparison  with  X.  In  the  pipes  examined  by  Zamminer, 
the  wave-length  X  varied  between  S4B  and  i^'6B.  In  the  first  case  the  loss 
would  be  T^T^th,  and  in  the  latter  about  ^th  of  the  amplitude. 

Now,  this  loss  of  vis  viva  can  be  replaced  in  various  ways.    Supposing  that 

%  the  small  volume  dV,  which  was  under  the  pressure  ^0*  ^^re  forced  over  into  a 
space  filled  with  air  under  the  pressure  p,  the  required  work  would  be  (p  -'PQ)dV. 
Hence  if  during  the  vibrations  of  sound,  at  those  places  and  times  where  the  air 
is  condensed,  either  a  small  quantity  of  air  is  regularly  forced  in,  or  the  pressiue 
of  the  compressed  air  is  increased  by  heating,  this  mass  of  air  generates  by  its 
expansion  a  greater  quantity  of  vis  viva  than  was  lost  by  its  resistance  to  the 
condensation  at  the  time  the  loss  occurred.  The  first  of  the  two  pauses  is  effective 
in  reed  pipes,  the  second  in  the  tubes  of  the  Pyrophone  [see  App.  XX.  sect.  N. 
No.  4] ,  In  which,  together  with  the  air  which  streams  back  into  the  tube,  an 
increased  quantity  of  gas  is  poured  in  firom  the  gas  tube,  and  this  on  burning 
increases  the  pressure  during  the  time  of  re^expansion. 

The  conditions  which  must  be  fulfilled  to  make  reed  pipes  speak  were  given  by 
me  in  the  '  Transactions  of  the  Association  for  Natural  History  and  Medicine ' 
(Verhandlungen  des  naturhistorischr^medicinischen  Vereins)  at  Heidelberg  (26  July 
i86j),  and  I  take  the  liberty  of  reprinting  this  short  explanation  here  with  a  few 

IT  improvementn. 

I.  The  Blowing  of  Bsed  Pipes. 

By  a  reed  pipe  I  mean  any  kind  of  wind  instrument  in  which  the  path  of  the 
stream  of  wind  is  alternately  opened  and  closed  by  means  of  a  vibrating  elastic 
body,  The  first  work  which  made  the  mechanics  of  reed  pipes  accessible  waa 
that  of  W,  Weber.  But  he  experimented  chiefly  with  metal  reeds,  which  on 
account  of  their  great  mass  and  elasticity,  could  not  be  powerfully  moved  by  the 
air  unless  the  tone  given  by  the  pipe  was  not  materiallv  different  from  the  proper 
tone  of  the  reed  independently  of  the  pipe.  Hence  pipes  with  metal  reeds  are 
usually  capable  of  producing  only  a  single  tone,  namely  that  one  among  those 
theoretically  possible  which  is  closest  to  the  proper  tone  of  the  reed. 

The  case  is  different  for  reeds  of  light  material  which  offers  but  little  resistance, 
such  as  the  cane  reeds  of  the  clarinet,  oboe,  bassoon,  and  the  muBcular  reeds  of 
the  human  lips  in  trumpets,  trombones,  and  horns,  Beeds  of  vulcanised  India* 
rubber,  placed  similarly  to  the  vocal  chords  in  the  larynx,  are  also  well  adapted  for 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


APP.  vu.  THEORY  OF  PIPES.  391 

experiments ;  but,  to  make  them  speak  easily  and  well,  care  must  be  taken  to 
place  them  obliquely  to  the  current  of  air  (p.  976). 

The  action  of  reeds  differs  essentially  according  as  the  passage  which  they  close 
is  opened  when  the  reed  moves  against  the  wind  towards  the  windchest,  or  moves 
with  the  wind  towards  the  pipe.  I  shall  say  that  the  first  strike  inwardsy  and  the 
second  strike  (mtwards.  The  reeds  of  the  clarinet,  oboe,  bassoon,  and  organ  all 
strike  inwards.  The  human  lips  in  brass  instruments,  on  the  other  hand,  are 
reeds  striking  outwards.  The  india-rubber  reeds  that  I  employ  may  be  arranged 
to  strike  either  inwards  or  outwards. 

The  laws  for  the  pitch  of  reed  pipes  are  completely  found,  when  we  have  deter- 
mined the  motion  of  the  reed  as  influenced  by  the  alternating  pressure  of  the  air 
in  the  pipe  and  air  chamber  [see  fig.  29,  B,  p  p,  on  p.  g6b] ;  remembering  that  the 
effluent  air  cannot  attain  its  maximum  velocity  until  the  passage  closed  by  the 
reed  has  been  ojpened  as  widely  as  possible. 

i)  Beeds  with  cyUndrical  pipe  without  air  chamber.  The  reed  is  regarded  as  ^ 
a  body  which  returns  to  its  position  of  equilibrium  by  elastic  forces,  and  is  again 
brought  out  of  that  position  by  the  pressure  of  the  aur  in  the  pipe,  which  changes 
periodically  with  the  sine  of  the  time.  The  equations  of  motion*  show  that  the 
instant  of  greatest  pressure  within  the  pipe  must  fall  between  a  maximum  dis- 
placement of  the  reed  outwards,  which  precedes  it,  and  a  maximum  displacement 
of  the  same  inwards,  which  follows  it.  If  the  vibrational  period  be  divided  into 
360®,  like  the  circumference  of  a  circle,  the  angle  c,  by  which  the  maximum  pres- 
sure precedes  the  passage  of  the  reed  through  its  position  of  equilibrium,  is  given 
by  the  equation 

tanc  =  ^'-A\ 

where  L  is  the  length  of  the  wave  of  the  reed  in  the  air  without  the  pipe,  X  the 
wave-length  of  the  tone  which  is  actually  produced,  and  /S^  a  constant  which  is 
greater  for  reeds  of  light  material  and  greater  friction  than  for  heavy  and  perfectly 
elastic  materials.     The  angle  c  must  be  taken  between  —  90^  and  +  90"^.  % 

In  the  same  way  we  must  determine  the  time  at  which  the  greatest  pressure 
within  the  pipe  separates  from  the  greatest  velocity.  The  latter  must  coincide 
with  the  position  of  the  reed  for  which  the  opening  is  greatest.  The  calculation 
of  this  magnitude  results  from  my  investigations  on  ^e  motion  of  air  within  an 
open  cylindrical  tube  {Jotimal  filr  reine  uvd  angewamdte  Mathematik,  Ivii.).  The 
maximum  of  the  velocity  in  the  direction  of  the  opening  precedes  the  maximum 
of  pressure  by  an  angle  8  (considering  the  vibrational  period  aa  the  cireumfsrence 
of  a  circle)  which  is  given  oy  the  equation 

where  S  is  the  transverse  section,  I  the  length  of  the  tnbe»  and  a  a  constant  de- 
pending on  the  form  of  the  opening,  being  45**  [but  see  note  f  and  t  V'  9}\  ^^^ 
cylindrical  tubes,  of  which  the  section  has  the  radius  p.  The  angle  8  in  this  case 
is  again  to  be  taken  between  —  90®  and  -h  90^. 

Now  as  air  can  only  enter  the  pipe  when  the  reed  leaves  the  passage  open,  it  f 
follows  that  for  reeds  which  strike  inwa/rds  the  maximum  velocity  of  the  air  dnrected 
outwards  must  coincide  with  tiie  maximum  displacement  of  the  teed  inwards. 
Hence  we  must  have 

2 

and  both  8  and  c  must  be  negative. 

For  reeds  which  strike  outwards^  on  the  other  hand,  the  maximiam  effluence  of 
the  air  must  coincide  with  the  maximum  di^lacement  of  the  reed  outwards,  and 
we  must  have 

_ir  =  8  +  €, 

2 

and  both  8  and  c  must  be  positive. 

*  To  be  treated  as  in  Seebeck*8  theory  of  the  following  App.  IX.  Bat  the  c  there  is  the 
sympathetic  resonance,  Bepertorium  der  Phy-  complement  of  &e  c  here,  and  wave-lengths  are 
ti/p,  vol.  vUi.  pp.  60-64.  Also  see  equation  4c  in       here  used  instead  of  pitch  numbers,  as  there. 


Digitized  by 


^.joogle 


392  THEORY  OF  PIPES.  app.  vu. 

Both  cases  are  included  in  the  equation 

tan  CSS  cot  S 


or 


8inlli^  =  ^S^.^    (,j 


in  which  the  reeds  must  strike  inwards  or  outwards  respectively,  according  as  the 
quantities  on  each  side  of  equation  (i)  are  positive  or  negative. 

Since  S  and  ^  are  very  small  quantities,  sin  {4^(2  -|-  a)  -i-X}  cannot  have  any 
sensible  value  unless  \*—L^  is  very  small,  that  is,  unless  the  pitch  of  the  mpe 
nearly  coincides  with  that  of  the  reed  sounded  separately  from  the  pipe.  This 
is  generally  the  case  with  metal  reeds.  The  viJue  of  X  is  determined  from 
equation  (i). 

On  the  contrary  when  the  difference  of  the  two  tones  X— 2/  is  great,  then 
f  sin  {4ir(Z  +  a)  -f-  X}  must  be  very  small,  and  hence  approzimatively 

4 

where  v  is  any  whole  number. 

The  alteration  of  pressure  within  the  tube  is  proportional  to  sin  {zw{l  +  a)-i-  X} , 
and  hence  is  a  minimum  when 


and  a  maximum  when 


4 


In  the  first  case  the  force  of  the  pressure  of  the  air  does  not  suffice  to  move  the 
m  reed.    In  the  second  case  it  suffices  when  the  reeds  are  not  too  heavy  and  have 
not  too  great  a  power  of  resistance.     Hence  the  tones  speak  well  for  which 
approzimatively 

Z  +  a  =  (2i^  +  i).^, 
4 

that  is,  for  which  the  column  of  air  in  the  pipe  vibrates  as  in  a  stopped  pipe.  At 
the  same  time  we  see  that  these  tones  are  almost  independent  of  the  proper  tone 
of  the  reed. 

Of  this  kind  are  the  tones  of  the  clarinet.  Membranous  reeds  of  india-rubber 
which  strike  inwards,  attached  to  glass  tubes  16  feet  long,  also  speak  easily  and 
allow  various  upper  partials  to  be  produced  which  agr^  well  with  equation  (i). 
The  reeds  which  strike  outwards  must  be  tuned  very  low  in  order  to  give  the 
pure  tones  of  the  tube.  Hence  the  human  lips  are  well  adapted  for  this  purpose, 
as  the  bundles  of  elastic  fibres  of  which  they  are  composed,  are  loaded  with  a 
IF  large  quantity  of  watery  inelastic  tissue  [see  footnote^  p.  97^.  Cylindrical  glass 
tubes  may  easily  be  blown  as  trumpets,  and  give  the  tones  of  a  stopped  pipe. 
Of  these  the  upper  tones,  for  which  the  difference  Zr^— X^  is  large,  can  be  pro- 
duced with  firmness  and  correct  intonation,  but  the  lower  tones,  on  the  other 
hand,  being  not  quite  independent  of  the  v^ue  of  L,  that  is,  of  the  tension  and 
density  of  the  lips,  are  uncertain  and  variable. 

2)  Beeds  with  conical  pipes  without  air  chamber.  There  is  a  remarkable 
difference  between  cylindrical  and  conical  pipes.  The  motion  of  the  air  in  the 
interior  of  the  latter  may  be  determined  on  the  same  principles  that  I  have  used 
for  cylindrical  pipes. 

Put  the  potential  of  the  motion  of  the  air,  inside  the  pipe,  equal  to 

^  {  Bin  p^ (jB-r  +  a)l  cos  2imt  j  +  ?^^? .  cos  ?^  {B-r) .  sin  2imt, 

where  r  is  the  distance  from  the  vertex  of  the  cone,  B  the  value  of  r  at  the  open- 
ing [or  base]  of  the  cone,  S  its  section,  a  the  difference  between  the  true  and 

Digitized  by  VjOOQlC 


JLPP,  vn. 


.THEORY  OF  PIPES. 


393 


reduced  length,  n  the  pitch  number.    Considering  a  -f-  X  to  be  very  small,  and 
putting  JS— r  =  i,  this  gives 


.        «        —X*       .     2ir(l  +  a) 

tan  h  = — -  .  sm      ^  —  '^J 


•  r« 


27r(Z  +  a) 


2Tr(l  4-  O^ 


,        -  ,  COS _ii^_i:i  +  _:l. sin  ""\'  "\ 

2irb  XL  X  27rr  A         J 


in  which  I  is  to  be  referred  to  the  place  of  the  reed. 
Here  also  we  have  to  put 

cot  ^  s=  tan  c. 

We  are  at  present  chiefly  interested  in  those  tones  of  pipe  which  differ  much 
from  the  tone  of  the  reed,  for  which  therefore  L^—P  is  great,  tan  €  is  also  con- 
sequently very  great,  and  tan  3  very  small.  For  these  then  we  must  either  have 
approximatively 

sin  [2ir{l  +  a)  -4-  X]  =  o, 

which  gives  no  tone  at  all,  because  the  alteration  of  pressure  in  the  interior  of  the  ^ 
pipe  is  too  weak,  or  else 

tan  [2ir(i  +  a)-i-X]  =  — 2irr-HX (2) 

This  is  the  equation  for  the  powerful  upper  tones  of  the  pipes. 

Below  is  the  series  of  tones  calculated  from  equation  (2)  for  a  conical  pipe  of 
zinc  of  the  following  dimensions  : 

Length  I  =  1297  centimetres  [  =  5i'o6  inch]. 

Diameter  of  the  openings  5*5  and  0*7  ctm.  [  =  2*17  and  '28  inch]. 

Reduced  length  Z  +  a,  calculated  I24'77  ctm.  [  =  49*12  inch]. 


Approximate 
Tone 

WaTe-Iengths 
calcuUiLed 

open                       j                     stopped 
pipes 

I.  B- 

centimetres 
28361  - 

centimetres 
f  X  141-80 

centimetres 
«  Jx    70-90 

2.  6- 

139-83  = 

1x139-84 

=  J  X  104-88 

3./'» 

91*81 -» 

|x  137-71 

-  }x  114-76 

4.6'  + 

6794  = 

ix  135-88 

=  fx  118-89 

5.  d'%  + 

5376- 

1 X  134-39 

«  1  X  I20-95 

6.  ^'- 

44.40=1 

Jx  133-21 

=  ^X  I22-II 

7.  ft^b- 

37-79- 

f  X  132-26 

»^x  122-82 

8.  c'"- 

3287  = 

|x  131-50 

=  ^x  123-28 

9.  d'"- 

29-22  » 

|x  131-47 

-^x  124-17 

The  tones  firom  the  2nd  to  the  9th  could  be  observed,  and  were  found  to  agree 
perfectly  with  the  calculation.  It  appears  from  the  last  two  columns  that  the 
higher  tones  were  almost  exactly  those  of  a  stopped  pipe,  the  length  of  which  is  ^ 
equal  to  the  reduced  length  of  the  pipe  124-77  ctm.,  and  that  the  deeper  tones 
approach  nearer  to  those  of  an  open  pipe,  the  length  of  which  was  that  from  the 
vertex  to  the  foot  of  the  cone.  The  reduced  length  of  this  would  be  JR  +  a=  142:6 
ctm.  [  =  56' 1 5  inch].  The  tones  of  brass  instruments  are  usually  assumed  to  be 
the  same  as  those  of  an  open  pipe,  but  the  higher  tones -of  these  instruments  are 
relatively  too  sharp*  for  the  lower  ones,  in  the  present  case  by  more  than  half 


*  [The  text  has  *  flat/  but  this  is  against 
the  figures.  As  it  will  appear  that  the  notes 
assigned  to  the  pitches  in  the  first  eolnmn 
are  only  roughly  approximative,  it  is  best  to 


calculate  out  the  intervals  in  cents,  and  as- 
suming that  the  pitch  varies  inversely  as  the 
wave-length,  we  hawe  in  cents — 


For  tones  . 

I 

0 
0 

0 

2 

3 

4 

5 

6 

7 

8 

9 

Reed  pipe  . 
Harmonics 

1221 
1200 

1953 
1902 

2474 
2400 

2879 
2786 

3210 
3102 

3489 
3369 

3731 
3600 

3935 
3804 

Difference  . 

21 

51 

74 

93 

108 

120 

Jii 

131 

Digitized  by  V^OOQ IC 


394 


THEORY  OP  PIPES. 


App.  vn. 


a  Tone.    In  trampets  and  horns  this  error  is  perhaps  io  some  extent  compensated 
by  the  caps  of  the  mouthpiece.    In  trombones  the  slides  assist.* 

Whereas  trampets,  trombones,  and  horns  belong  to  the  reed  pipes  of  this 
class  with  conical  pipes,  and  deep  reeds  which  strike  outwards,  oboes  and  bas- 
soons have  high  reeds  which  strike  inwards.  When  strongly  blown  they  also 
give  the  higher  Octave  and  then  the  Twelfth,  like  an  open  pipe.  The  calculation 
from  equation  (2)  for  the  oboe  agrees  very  well  with  Zamminer's  measurements. 
[Zam.  ibid,  p.  306.] 

n.   The  Bloiving  of  Flue  Pipes. 

In  my  memoir  on  *  The  Discontinuous  Motions  of  Fluids  *  (Monthly  Proceed- 
*    ings  of  the  Academy  of  Sciences  at  Berlin,  April  23,  1868},  I  nave  described  the 
mechanical  peculiarities  of  such  motions,  and  deduced  from  the  theory  how  ihey 
are  brought  about  by  means  of  the  blade-shaped  current  of  air  at  the  mouth  of  an 
m  organ  pipe  which  is  blown,  as  described  on  p.  92a  to  p.  93a.     The  bounding  surfaces 
,    of  this  current  which  cuts  through  and  across  the  mass  of  air  that  runs  into  and 
out  of  the  mouth  of  the  pipe,  are  to  be  considered  as  vortical  surfaces,  that  is, 
surfaces  which  are  faced  with  a  continuous  stratum  of  vortical  filaments  or  thread- 
like eddies.     Such  surfe^es  have  a  very  unstable  equilibrium.     An  infinitely  ex- 
tended plane  surfEM^  uniformly  covered  with  parallel  straight  vortical  filaments 
might  indeed  continue  stable ;  but  where  the  least  flexure  occurs  at  any  time, 
the  surface  curls  itself  round  in  ever  narrowing  spiral  coils,  which  continually 
involve  more  and  more  distant  parts  of  the  surface  in  their  vortex. 


shewing  that  the  tones  of  the  reed  pipe  are 
always  too  sharp,  and  not  too  flat,  as  the 
German  text  states,  the  sharpness  being  a 
comma  for  tone  2,  a  Quartertone  for  tone  3, 
^  Tone  for  4,  nearly  a  Semitone  for  5,  more 
than  a  Semitone  for  the  rest,  the  last  two 
being  equally  too  sharp  by  aboat  1}  Semitone. 
%  The  misprint  of  {2'"8  tor  d'"  in  the  German  text 

For  tones 


made  the  last  tone  appear  to  be  a  whole  l\ne 
too  sharp.  In  determining  the  notes  in  column 
I,  the  Author  has  probably  assumed  the  velo- 
city, of  sound  at  342  metres  (which  gives  11 22 
feet,  or  the  velocity  at  about  60'  F.,  see  note 
p.  god),  and  divided  it  by  the  wave-lengths 
reduced  to  metres.    This  would  give — 


I 

2 

3 

4 

5 

6 

7 

8 

1 206 
1246 

244-6 
249-2 

372-5 
373-5 

5034 
498-4 

6362 
628-0 

770*3 
781-6 

905-0 
9400 

1040-5 
10560 

B 

b 

ft 

b' 

d'% 

9"' 

6"b 

&" 

the  pitch  nos. 
While  pitch  nos.    . 
belong  to  equally  \^ 
tempered  notes  J 

Whence  it  appears  that  g"  and  b"\>  are  much, 
and  cf'\  d'"  somewhat,  too  sharp,  so  that  the 
d"%  of  the  German  text  is  a  manifest  error. 
This  rough  mode  of  comparing  by  vibrational 


1170-4. 
1 184-2 

d'" 


numbers,  does  not  however  convey  a  proper 
conception.  If  we  calculate  the  cents  from 
C66,  we  find— 


For  tones 

I 

2 

3 

4 

5 

6 

7 

8 

1   ' 

the  cents 
But  cents 

belong  to  equally  1 
m     tempered  notes  J 

1044 

IIOO 

B 

2268 
2300 

b 

2996 
3000 

n 

3517 
3500 

V 

3923 
3900 

d"i 

4254 
4300 

4600 
6"b 

4774 
4800 

4978. 
5000 

d" 

Differences 

-56 

-32 

-4 

+  17 

+  23 

-46 

-67 

-26 

-22. 

And  this  shews  how  very  rough  are  the  ap- 
proximations to  the  pitch  which  are  made  in 
the  text  by  means  of  equally  tempered  notes. — 
TramlaUyr^ 

♦  [*  The  conical  tube  examined  by  Prof. 
Helmholtz,*  says  Mr.  Blaikley  (see  note^ 
p.  97<2),  *wa8  not  a  perfect  but  a  truncated 
cone,  and  any  such  would  have  its  series  of 
intervals  intermediate  between  i,  2,  3,  4,  5, 
&c.,  and  I,  3,  5,  7,  9,  <&c. ;  that  is,  a  perfect 
cone,  or  one  truncated  to  an  infinitely  small 
extent,  would  have  the  first,  and  an  infinitely 
long  cone  (  »  a  stopped  cylindrical  tube) 
would  have  the  second.  Such  a  cone  as 
Helmholtz  describes  is  wit  a  representative  of 
the  brass  instrument  family,  for  if  cylindrical 


tubing  were  added  at  the  small  end,  the  series 
with  this  added  tube  would  not  even  be  so 
near  the  theoretical  i,  2,  3,  4,  5,  <J^o.,  as  on  the 
original  cone.  There  are  brass  instruments  in 
which  the  series,  so  far  from  getting  sharp  on 
the  higher  tones,  gets  flat,  i,  -  2, — 3, — 4,  >-  5,^fco. 
Technically  such  instruments  are  said  to  be 
**  sharp  at  the  bottom.**  In  short,  trumpets 
and  trombones,  <&c.,  are  not  conical  in  the 
ordinary  sense  of  the  word,  but  have  in  most 
cases  a  cylindrical  tube  expanding  into  a  bell 
by  a  line  of  increasing  curvature,  so  that  the 
boundaries  are  approximately  hyperbolic* 
MS.  communication.  See  also  note  f*  P-  99^ 
— TraiiilaU>r,'\   . 


Digitized  by  VjOOQIC 


APP.  VII.  THEORY  OP  PIPES.  395 

This  inclination  of  the  dividing  surfaces  of  masses  of  air  when  moved  discon- 
tinuously,  to  resolve  themselves  into  vortices,  can  also  be  clearly  seen  on  cylin- 
drical streams  of  air,  driven  from  cylindrical  pipes  and  mixed  with  a  little  smoke 
to  make  them  visible.  In  perfectly  quiet  air  and  under  favourable  conditions,  they 
may  reach  a  length  of  one  foot  to  three  feet.  The  least  noise  however  makes 
them  shrink  up,  as  the  vortices  then  commence  close  to  their  origin.  Professor 
TyndaU  has  also  observed  and  described  a  great  number  of  similar  phenomena  of 
this  kind,  in  burning  gas  jets.* 

This  resolution  into  vortices  takes  place  in  the  blade  of  air  at  the  mouth  of 
the  pipe,  where  it  strikes  against  the  lip.  From  this  place  on  it  is  resolved  into 
vortices,  and  thus  mixes  with  the  surrounding  oscillating  air  of  the  pipe,  and 
accordingly  as  it  streams  inwards  or  outwards,  it  reinforces  its  inward  or  outward 
velocity,  and  hence  acts  as  an  accelerating  force  with  a  periodically  alternating 
direction,  which  turns  from  one  side  to  the  other  with  great  rapidity.  Such  a 
blade  of  air  follows  the  transversal  oscillations  of  the  surrounding  mass  of  air  ^ 
without  sensible  resistance.  During  the  phase  of  entrance  of  air,  the  blade  is 
also  directed  inwards,  and  thus  on  its  part  reinforces  the  vis  viva  of  the  inward 
currents.    Conversely,  for  the  outward  current.f 

If  we  suppose  the  accelerating  force  of  the  current  of  air  to  be  represented  by 
one  of  Fourier's  series,  the  amplitude  of  any  term  of  the  order  m  will  in  general 
diminish  as  i  -nm  (see  p.  35^2).  In  fact  we  require  only  to  use  the  expreRsion 
given  in  App.  HI.  p.  375,  equations  (ib)  and  (3)  for  the  displacement  ^  of  a 

plucked  string,  in  order  to  find  the  value  of  -^,  for  the  time  ^  =  o.    We  thus  find  the 

ax 

series  which  expresses  the  periodical  alternation  between  a  greater  and  smaller 

value  of  y,  as  shewn  ia  fig.  19,  p.  54c. 

From  my  memoir  *  On   the  Vibrations  of  Air  in  Tubes  with  Open  Ends,' 

already  cited  (p.  388a),  it  follows  that  throughout  the  tube  a  positive  component  of 

pressure  coincides  with  the  maximum  velocity  in  the  direction  of  the  opening,  and 

when  multiplied  by  such  velocity  this  component  has  the  value  % 

aA^k^S 

where 

a  is  the  velocity  of  sound, 

A  the  maximum  velocity  at  the  end  of  the  tube, 

S  the  transverse  section  of  the  cylindrical  portion  of  the  tube, 

k  =  27r  -i-  \,  X  being  the  length  of  the  wave. 

If,  then,  two  trains  of  waves  start  from  any  transverse  section  in  the  directions  of 
the  two  ends,  and  have  the  same  velocity  at  that  section,  the  above  component 
of  pressure  must  be  directed  in  opposite  ways  in  the  two  trains  of  waves.  This 
holds  for  the  place  of  blowing  even  when  it  is  quite  close  to  the  end  of  the  tube,  so 
that  one  train  of  waves  is  infinitesimally  short.  Under  these  circumstances  the 
acceleration  produced  by  the  air  blown  in,  must  correspond  to  twice  that  difference 
of  pressure.  Since  A  is  the  velocity  at  the  opening,  twice  this  difference  of  pres-  H 
sure  for  the  mtii  tone,  wiQ  be 

a  A^  2ir  Sm^ 


This  would  be  the  only  difference  of  pressure  if  the  tone  blown  exactly  corre- 
sponded to  the  proper  tone  of  the  tube.  But  it  may  be  shewn  that  this  cannot  be 
made  to  agree  with  the  mechanism  of  blowing,  and  there  is  always  a  length  fi 
which  must  be  intercalated  between  the  two  trains  of  waves  in  order  to  reduce 

*  J.  TyndaU  On  Sounds  Lect.  VI. ,  also,  in  Journal  January,  1867 ;  Nature^  vol.  viii.  pp. 

Philosophical  Magassine,  series  iv.  vol.  xxxiii.  25,  45,  383,  vol.  x.  pp.  161,  481,  vol.  xi.  p.  325 

pp.  92-99,  and  375-391.  [24  Jane   1875,  P-  »45 '»   27  April   1876,  p. 

f  The  formation  of  this  blade  of  air  has  Sn])*    Herr  Sohneebeli  also  gave  a  mechani- 

been  described  by  Messrs.  Schneebeli  (Pogg.,  cal  explanation  of  the  principal  features  of 

Ann.  cliii.  p.  301),  Sonreck  (ib.  clviii.  p.  129),  the  process.     [See  the  Translator's  addition 

and  Hermann    Smith    (English   Mechanics^  at  the  end  of  this  Appendix,  p.  3966.] 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


396 


THEORY   OF  PIPES. 


APP.  VTL 


them  to  an  accordant  series  of  constant  vibrations.    lu  this  case  there  is  another 
additional  difference  of  pressure  equal  to 


—a  4^  sin 


2'frmP 


For  the  smaller  numbers  expressing  the  order,  the  sine  may  be  replaced  by 
the  arc,  and  this  latter  term  considered  as  the  greater.  Consequently  the  lower 
partials  of  the  musical  tone  produced,  allow  the  coefficient  m  A^io  increase  as 
i-7-m,  that  is  A^  as  i  -f-  m^,  and  the  higher  partials  allow  A^  to  increase  as 
I  ^  rn^.  The  velocities  of  the  partial  vibrations  in  more  distant  parts  of  the 
external  free  air  contain  the  factor  k  once  more  than  the  velocities  in  the  tubes 
(see  equations  i2g  and  i2h  in  my  memoir).  These  will  consequently  increase  as 
I  -t-  m  for  lower  values  of  m,  which  is  also  the  case  for  the  velocities  of  violin 
strings,  but  for  higher  values  of  m  they  decrease  as  i  h-  m*.  The  greater  S  is,  the 
f  sooner  will  this  more  considerable  decrease  of  the  higher  partials  occur.  It  is 
for  this  reason  especiaUy  that  organ-builders  compare  the  tones  of  narrow  metal 
organ  pipes  with  those  of  the  violin  and  violoncello. 

The  circumstances  which  affect  the  blowing  of  pipes  and  the  value  of  the  mag- 
nitude P,  require  a  more  extended  investigation,  which  I  hope  to  be  soon  aUe  to 
give  elsewhere. 

Additions  bt  Tbanslator. 


[It  may  be  conTenient  for  those  not  con- 
versant with  mathematics  to  reproduce  the 
aoconnt  of  the  phenomena,  which  was  given  by 
me  in  pp.  708-711  of  the  ist  English  edition. 

Uerr  Sohneebeli  had  an  experimental  pipe 
constructed  in  the  usual  way  ;  with  glass  back 
and  a  movable  lip  and  slit  or  windway,  through 
which  was  driven  air  impregnated  with  smoke, 
as  is  frequently  done  to  make  it  visible.    When 

^  he  so  placed  the  lip  and  slit  that  the  stream 
of  air  passed  entirely  otUside  of  the  pipe,  no 
sound  occurred;  but  if  he  blew  gently  upon 
this  sheet  of  air,  at  right  angles,  the  pipe 
sounded,  and  the  tone  continued  until  he  blew 
through  the  other  end  of  the  pipe ;  neverthe- 
less under  these  circumstances  it  was  very 
rare  indeed  to  find  that  any  smoke  penetrated 
into  the  pipe.  If  the  sheet  of  smoked  air 
passed  entirely  inside  of  the  pipe,  there  was 
also  no  sound ;  but  then  on  blowing  through 
the  open  end,  so  as  to  force  some  of  it  out, 
sound  was  produced,  and  it  was  stopped  by 
blowing  against  the  slit.  This  case  was  there- 
fore the  converse  of  the  last.  He  concludes : 
*That  the  stream  of  air  which  issues  from 
the  slit  forms  a  species  of  air-reed  (Luft- 
Lamelle,  aerial  lamina),  and  that  this  plays 
in  the  generation  of  vibrations  in  the  mass  of 

H  air,  a  part  analogous  to  that  of  reeds  in  reed 
pipes.*  He  states  that  the  vibrational  nature 
of  motion  of  the  air  between  the  slit  and  the 
lip  can  be  shewn  by  attaching  a  piece  of  silk- 
paper  to  the  edge  of  the  lip  or  the  split,  and 
pressing  a  point  against  it.  He  further  pro- 
poses a  theory  founded  on  Helmholtz's  hydro- 
dynamical  investigations  in  the  Berichte  der 
Berliner  Akademie^  1868,  23  April,  Crelle  60, 
and  states  it  to  this  effect :  When  the  split  is 
in  the  normal  position  the  air-reed  strikes  the 
lip,  a  portion  of  the  stream  enters,  and  pro- 
duces a  compression  as  in  reed  pipes;  the 
reaction  of  this  compression  affects  the  air- 
reed  and  bends  it  outwards  ;  on  the  pressure 
ceasing  the  air-reed  returns  to  its  original 
position  and  tiie  process  begins  afresh. 

Mr.  Hermann  Smith  states  that  the  air 
from  the  bellows  is  iwt  directed  '  against  the 


edge  of  the  lip,*  and  that,  if  it  were  so  directed, 
the  pipe  would  not  speak.  He  also  states  that 
the  sharpness  of  the  lip  is  immaterial  to  mere 
speaking,  and  that  a  pipe  that  speaks  well 
may  have  the  edge  of  its  lip  half  an  inch 
thick.  (Compare  supril,  p.  6qc,  where  the 
wind  which  excites  the  sound  in  the  bottle 
is  blown  across  its  mouth  and  the  edges  of 
the  opening  are  rounded,  not  sharps) 

The  source  of  tone,  according  to  both  Mr. 
Hermann  Smith  and  Herr  Schneebeli,  is  the 
formation  of  what  the  former  calls  an  ^  aero- 
plastic  reed,*  and  also  simply  an  '^air-reed,* 
and  the  latter  a  *  Luft-Lamelle  *  (Prof.  Helm- 
holtz's *  Luft-Blatt,*  air-blade,  oi;  blattfdrmige 
Schicht, '  blade-shaped  stratum  or  sheet,*  as 
used  BupriL,  p.  394a),  which  is  produced  out- 
side of  the  pipe,  and  bends  partly  within 
it.  For  the  formation  of  this  reed  both  agree 
that  it  is'  essential  for  the  exciting  air  to  pass 
the  lip,  certainly  not  to  enter  the  pipe.  The 
existence  of  the  reed  is  shewn  by  Mr.  Her- 
mann Smith  by  interposing  a  thin  lamina,  a 
shaving,  or  crisp  tissue  paper,  which  is  caught 
by  the  air  and  vibrates  as  a  reed*,  and  by  Herr 
Schneebeli  by  the  smoke  mixed  with  air  which 
enables  the  experimenter  to  see  its  motion 
directly,  and  also  by  a  piece  of  silk-paper. 

Herr  Schneebeli  supposes  this  air -reed  to 
act  by  producing  condensation,  but  Mr.  Her- 
mann Smith's  theory  of  its  origin  seems  to 
be  as  follows.  The  air  driven  rapidly  and 
closely  from  the  slit  past  the  mouth  of  the 
pipe,  in  a  flat  stream,  just  and  only  just 
avoiding  the  edge  of  the  lip,  creates  a  vacuum, 
precisely  as  in  the  tubes  for  ether  spray  or 
perfume  spray  in  common  use,  or  in  ordinary 
chimney-pots.  The  air  in  the  pipe  under 
the  action  of    the  atmospheric  pressure  at 

•  Press  a  piece  of  crisp,  bat  very  light,  thin  paper 
firmly  Against  the  oatalde  of  the  windway  by  moans  oC 
a  card  or  piece  of  ynxA.  Let  the  paper  project  upwards 
till  it  nearly  oorers  the  mouth,  but  is  quite  dear  of 
both  Up  and  ears.  The  paper  then  resembles  a  free 
reed.  On  now  blowing  in  the  u^nal  way  through  the 
slit,  the  pajier  wi'.l  commence  its  Tibrations  which  Mr. 
Hermann  Smith  ponsidcfi  to  correspond  to  thoae  of  his 
air-rocd.    The  eilect  is  caciiy  observed. 


Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


.APP.  VII. 


THEORY  OF  PIPES. 


397 


the  upper  open  end  immediately  descends  to 
supply  this  vacumn,  and  by  so  doing  not 
<only  bends  the  flat  exciting  stream  of  air 
outwards,  but  also  of  course  produces  a  rare- 
faction in  the  tube,  which  by  extending  from 
the  mouth  upwards  necessarily  weakens  the 
force  of  the  outward  rush  of  wind.  The  ex- 
ternal (not  the  exciting)  air,  taking  advantage 
of  this  relaxation  of  force,  enters  the  tube  at 
the  lip,  causing  a  condensation  in  the  lower 
part  of  the  pipe,  and  the  resulting  wave  of 
condensation  before  it  has  proceeded  half-way 
meets  the  former  wave  of  rarefaction,  which 
continued  to  proceed  from  the  further  end  of 
the  tube,  and  thus  forms  a  node.  The  node 
is  consequently  always  nearer  the  mouth  than 
the  end  of  the  pipe.  The  ratio  of  the  length 
of  the  segment  further  from  the  mouth  to 
that  nearer  to  it  varies  from  4  :  3  to  7  :  6  ac- 
cording to  the  diameter  or  scale  of  the  pipe 
and  the  strength  of  wind  (see  p.  91 6,  and 
footnotes  t,  %).  In  the  meantime  the  exciting 
stream  of  air  rights  itself,  passes  over  the 
vertical,  bends  inwards,  and  a  small  portion 
of  it  enters  the  pipe  with  the  external  air,  to 
be  cast  out  again  by  the  returning  wave  of 
rarefaction,  and  by  this  time  the  exciting 
stream  of  air  has  been  converted  into  a  vibra- 
ting air-reed.  That  the  first  (but  momentary) 
effect  of  the  upward  rush  of  the  exciting 
stream  of  air  is  to  abstract  air  from  the  pipe 
Mr.  Hermann  Smith  considers  to  be  demon- 
strated by  inserting  on  the  'languid,'  just 
within  the  mouth,  some  filaments  of  cotton, 
fluff,  or  down,  which,  in  larger  pipes,  are  shot 
out  with  energy.  He  supposes  the  air-reed 
afterwards  to  abstract  and  admit  air  in  con- 
stant succession,  thus  producing  the  necessaiy 
stimulu»  for  the  sound  heard,  which  would  on 
this  theory  depend,  among  other  conditions, 
upon  the  force  of  blast,  its  inclination  to  the 
mouth,  the  size  and  form  of  the  mouth  and 
ears,  the  interposition  of  obstructions  between 
the  reed  and  outer  air  (as  the  shading  bar  of 
the  Oamba),  the  capacity  and  length  of  the 
pipe,  and  whether  a  node  has  to  be  formed  in 
the  pipe  in  the  mode  explained  for  open  pipes, 
or  in  the  mode  used  for  stopped  pipes,  which 
are  acknowledged  to  speak  more  readily  than 
the  former.  The  external  air  of  course  passes 
continually,  but  intermittently,  tiirough  tiie 
mouth  of  the  pipe. 

The  law  of  vibration  of  the  air-reed  as 
stated  by  Mr.  Hermann  Smith,  but  unob* 
served  and  possibly  unobtained  by  Herr  Schnee- 
beli,  is :  *  As  its  arcs  of  vibration  are  less, 
its  speed  is  preater,*  6r  *  the  times  {of  vibra- 
tion) vary  with  the  amplittidet*  being  different 
from  the  usual  law  of  a  vibrating  reed  in 
which  the  time  is  independent  of  the  ampli- 
tude of  vibration.  If  by  extraneous  influence 
the  pitch  of  the  pipe  is  flattened,  as  by  partly 
shading  the  mouth  from  the  external  air,  Mr. 
Hermann  Smith  states  that  the  path  of  the 
air-reed  is  lengthened,  and  conversely.  When 
an  organ  pipe  speaks  the  tones  of  the  air- 
reed  and  pipe  are  distinct  and  may  be  sepa- 
rated or  combined;  and  when  a  pipe  is  said 
*  to  fly  off  to  its  Octave,*  hd  says  that  the  air- 


reed  leaps  back  to  its  Octave  speed,  compel- 
ling the  pipe  to  follow,  and  that  this  can  be 
made  visible.  The  natural  pitch  of  the  air- 
reed  is  also,  he  states,  far  higher  than  the 
pitch  of  the  tones  of  the  pipe. 

Herr  Sonreck's  account,  although  tho- 
roughly independent,  agrees  with  Mr.  Her- 
mann Smith's  so  closely  as  regards  the  origin 
of  the  motion  of  air  in  the  pipe  that  it  seems 
unnecessary  to  cite  it.  He  calls  the  blade  of 
air  merely  the  Anblasestrom,  i.e.  *  blow-cur- 
rent '  or  '  blast.' 

But  so  far  as  I  have  hitherto  found,  the 
first  person  who  drew  attention  to  the  mode  in 
which  flue  pipes  were  made  to  speak,  was  M. 
Aristide  Cavaill6-Coll,  the  celebrated  French 
organ-builder,  who  in  a  paper  presented  to  the 
French  Academy  of  Sciences  on  February  24, 
1840  (which  was  never  printed,  owing  appa-  IT 
rently  to  the  death  of  M.  Savart,  one  of  the 
referees),  *  demonstrated,'  as  he  states  in  his 
paper  printed  in  the  Comptes  Rendus  for  i860 
(even  then  anterior  to  all  the  other  writers), 
vol.  1.  p.  176,  'more  dearly  than  had  hitherto 
been  done,  the  real  function  of  the  originator 
of  the  sound  in  the  mouth  of  flue  pipes,  which 
originator  he  assimilated  to  a  free  aerial  reed 
(anche  Ubre  airienney  He  also  investigated 
the  mode  of  blowing  the  flute,  by  the  mouth 
or  a  mouthpiece.  And  lastly  he  treated  of 
the  analogy  between  'the  transversal  vibra- 
tions of  vibrating  lamine  of  air,  and  solid 
vibrating  laminiB,'  which  he  supposed  to  be 
governed  by  the  same  laws,  and  from  this 
examination  he  deduced  '  positive  data  for 
determining  the  height  of  mouths  of  flue  pipes 
in  relation  to  their  intonation  and  the  eUstic  f 
force  of  air  which  excites  them.'  He  informs 
me  that  he  intends  to  publish  this  paper. 

In  conclusion,  it  diould  be  observed  that 
this  blade  or  flat  current  of  air  acts  like  a 
metal  reed  only  so  far  as  it  oscillates  back- 
wards and  forwards,  and  not  in  other  respects. 
It  does  not  shut  off  and  open  out  a  stream  of 
air  alternately.  It  is  moved  inwards  by  the 
outward  air,  and  outwards  by  the  inclosed  air, 
whereas  the  metal  reed  is  moved  only  one  way 
by  the  current  of  air,  and  the  other  way  by 
its  own  elasticity,  l^ere  is  therefore  simply 
an  analogy  and  not  a  substantive  similarity, 
so  that  tiie  terms  aero-plastic  reed,  air-reed, 
free  aerial  reed,  suggesting  a  different  opera- 
tion to  what  actually  ensues,  might  be  disused 
with  advantage.  The  action  seems  really  to 
be  one  of  alternate  rarefaction  and  condensa- 
tion. But  there  are  numerous  litUe  points  IT 
which  require  veiy  careful  study — ^the  shape 
of  the  upper  lip;  straight  (as  usual)  or  arched 
(as  in  Benatus  Harris's  flue  pipes),  the  height 
of  the  opening  of  the  mouth,  the  exact  direc- 
tion of  the  blade  of  air  in  relation  to  that  of  the 
upper  lip,  the  presence  and  shape  of  the  ears, 
and  the  general  arts  of  the  *  voicer '  whereby 
he  makes  a  pipe  '  speak '  satisfactorily.  All  of 
these  matters  influence  both  pitch  and  quality 
of  tone,  and  though  they  are  daily  practised, 
their  theory  is  as  good  as  unknown. 

For  observations  on  the  action  of  reeds  see 
App.  XX.  sect.  N.  No.  8.] 


Digitized  by 


Google 


398  EXPERBIENTS  ON  COMPOSITION  OP  VOWELS,      app.  viil 


APPENDIX  Vm. 

PBACTICAIi  DIBECTIONS  FOB  PBBPOBMINQ  THE  EXPEBIMBNTS  ON  THE 
COMPOSITION  OF  VOWELS. 

(See  p.  1 2  2d.) 

To  make  the  forks  vibrate  powerfully,  it  is  necessary  that  the  ratios  of  their  piteh 
numbers  should  agree  with  the  simple  arithmetics^  ratios  to  the  utmost  mcety. 
Afber  the  forks  have  been  toned  by  the  maker  by  ear  and  to  the  piano  as  accu- 
rately as  is  possible  in  this  way,  the  necessary  greater  exactness  is  obtained  by  the 
electrical  current  itself.  First  the  interrupting  fork,  fig.  33,  p.  122c,  is  connected 
with  the  fork  that  gives  the  prime  tone,  and  the  movable  clamp  on  the  former  is 

^  arranged  so  as  to  make  the  unison  perfect.  This  gives  a  maxunum  intensity  to 
the  prime  tone,  easily  recognised  by  both  epre  and  ear.  The  vibrations  of  this 
lowest  fork  are  so  powerful  that  the  excursions  of  the  extremities  of  the  prongs 
under  favourable  circumstances  amount  to  2  or  3  millimetres  (from  '08  to  *i2 
inch).  It  should  also  be  observed  that  if  the  unison  has  not  been  perfectly  at- 
tained, a  few  beats  of  the  fork  are  heard  when  the  electric  current  is  first  brought 
to  bear  upon  it,  although  these  disappear  when  the  apparatus  is  in  full  action. 
[This  is  accounted  for  in  Appendix  IX.] 

After  perfect  unison  has  oeen  accomplished  between  the  interrupting  fork  and 
that  of  the  prime  tone,  the  other  forks  are  successively  brought  into  electrical  con- 
nection, wiui  their  resonance  chambers  wide  open,  and  they  are  tuned  until  they 
reach  a  maximum  intensity  when  excited  by  the  current.  The  tuning  is  first  per- 
formed wiUi  the  file.  The  forks  are  sharpened,  as  is  well  known,  bv  taking  off 
some  metal  from  their  extremities,  and  flattened  by  reducing  the  thickness  of  the 
root  of  the  prongs.  Both  must  be  done  with  the  greatest  possible  uniformity  to 
each  prong.    To  discover  whether  the  fork  is  too  sharp  or  too  flat,  stick  a  httle 

f  piece  of  wax  at  the  ends  of  its  prongs  (which  flattens  the  fork)  and  observe  whether 
the  tone  becomes  louder  or  weaker.  If  louder,  the  fork  was  too  sharp ;  if  weaker, 
it  was  too  flat.  Since  alterations  of  temperature,  and,  perhaps,  other  causes  exert 
a  slight  influence  on  the  pitch  of  the  forks,  I  have  preferred  to  make  the  higher 
forks  a  little  too  sharp  by  filing,  and  to  bring  them  into  exact  tune  by  attaching 
small  quantities  of  wax  to  the  extremities  of  the  prongs.  The  quantity  of  wax  is 
easily  altered  at  pleasure,  and  by  this  means  shght  accidental  variations  of  pitch 
can  be  readily  corrected. 

It  is  not  necessary  to  tune  the  resonance  tubes  so  accurately ;  if,  when  blown 
across,  they  give  the  same  pitch  as  the  forks,  the;^  are  sufiiciently  well  tuned.  If 
they  are  too  flat,  some  melted  wax  may  be  poured  in  to  sharpen  them.  If  they  are 
too  sharp,  the  opening  must  be  reduced. 

It  cost  me  some  trouble  to  get  rid  of  the  noise  of  the  spark  at  the  point  of  in- 
terruption. At  first  I  inserted  a  large  condenser  of  tin  foil  such  as  is  used  in 
induction  machines.  But  this  merely  reduced  the  spark  to  a  certain  size.  No 
%  good  effect  followed  from  increasing  the  size  of  the  condenser.  The  lasers  of  the 
condenser  are  separated  by  thin  varnished  paper ;  one  is  connected  with  the  in- 
terrupting fork,  and  the  other  with  the  cup  of  quicksilver  into  which  its  end  dips. 
After  many  vain  attempts,  I  at  last  found  that,  by  inserting  a  very  long  and  thin 
wire  between  the  two  extremities  of  the  conduction  at  the  point  of  interruption^ 
the  noise  of  the  spark  was  almost  entirely  destroyed,  without  injuring  the  action 
of  the  current  on  the  forks.  The  wire  thus  inserted  must  have  an  amount  of 
resistance  far  greater  than  that  occasioned  by  the  coils  in  all  the  electro-magnets 
taken  together.  When  this  is  the  case  no  sensible  portion  of  the  current  wUl  go 
through  tliis  wire.  It  is  not  till  the  conduction  is  broken  and  the  thin  wire  forms 
the  ouly  connection  for  the  extra  current  of  the  electro-magnets,  that  the  current 
discharges  itself  through  the  wire.  But  to  prevent  the  thin  wire  itself  from 
generating  any  secondary  current,  it  must  not  be  coiled  round  a  cylinder,  but  must 
be  stretched  up  and  down  on  a  board  in  such  a  way  that  two  adjacent  pieces  of  the 
wire  should  be  traversed  by  two  currents,  proceeding  in  opposite  directions.  For 
this  purpose  I  screwed  two  hard  india-rubber  combs  at  the  two  ends  of  a  board 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


APP.  VIII.      EXPERIMENTS  ON  COMPOSITION  OF  VOWELS. 


399 


(one  foot  long),  and  passed  a  thin  plated  copper  wire,  such  as  is  used  for  spinning 
over  with  silken  threads,  backwards  and  forwards  (90  times)  between  the  teeth  of 
^ese  combs.  By  this  means  a  great  length  (90  feet)  of  this  wire  was  brought 
i^ell  insulated  into  a  comparatively  small  space,  in  such  a  way  as  not  to  produce 
any  sensible  secondary  current.  For  when  on  breaking  the  primary  current  a 
secondary  current  would  be  formed  in  the  wire,  this  would  have  a  direction  in  the 
circuit  formed  by  the  electro-magnets  and  the  thin  wire,  opposite  to  the  secondary 
current  in  the  electro-magnets,  and  the  latter  would  be  entirely  or  partly  pre- 
vented from  discharging  itself  through  the  thin  wire. 

For  moving  the  forks  I  used  two  or  three  cells  of  a  Orove's  battery.  The 
electro-magnets  were  placed  in  two  rows  beside  one  another.  The  whole  arrange- 
ment is  shewn  in  a  diagram  in  fig.  64,  below.  The  figures  i  to  8  shew  the 
resonant  chambers  of  the  tuning-forks ;  the  dotted  lines  which  lead  to  mi  and  mg 
are  the  threads  which  remove  the  cover  from  the  opening  of  the  resonance  . 
chambers ;  ai  to  ag  are  the  electro-magnets  which  set  in  motion  the  tuning-forks  f 


Fig.  64. 


tar^ 


m    SH^ 


-  —  my 


^^^ 


If 


between  their  legs ;  h  is  the  interrupting  fork,  and  f  its  own  electro-magnet.     The  % 
relative  position  of  the  two  last  has  been  somewhat  changed  in  order  to  make  the 
connection  of  the  direction  of  the  currents  more  intelligible.    The  cells  of  the 
galvanic  battery  are  marked  Ci  and  e, ;  the  great  resistance-wire  dd ;  the  condenser 
c,  but  its  plates  which  are  rolled  in  a  spiral  are  seen  only  in  section. 

The  electric  current  passes  from  e^,  through  all  the  electro-magnets  in  order, 
up  to  the  handle  of  the  interrupting  fork  g.  It  is  sometimes  more  advantageous 
to  arrange  this  part  of  the  conduction  so  that  it  should  be  separated  into  two 
parallel  branches,  and  that  the  three  highest  forks,  which  are  the  most  difficult  to 
set  in  motion,  should  be  inserted  into  one  branch,  and  the  five  lower  forks  into  the 
other,  thus  allowing  a  stronger  stream  to  pass  through  the  former  than  the  latter. 

The  remainder  of  the  conduction  from  g  to  the  second  pole  of  the  battery  Ci 
contains  the  interrupting  apparatus,  which  is  here  so  arranged  that  each  vibration 
of  ttie  fork  makes  the  current  twice ;  once  when  the  upper  prong  dips  into  the 
cup  of  mercury  h,  and  once  when  the  lower  prong  dips  into  the  cup  i.  When 
the  conduction  is  closed  at  h,  it  passes  from  g  through  the  fork  to  h,  and  then 

Digitized  by  V^ O OQ IC 


400     PHASES  OF  WAVES  CAUSED  BY  RESONANCE,  app.  vm.  re. 

through  the  electro-magnet  f  to  k  and  e^  Between  h  and  k  it  is  generally  neces- 
sary to  insert  a  lateral  branch  h  1  k,  having  moderate  resisting  power,  to  weaken 
the  current  in  the  eleotro-magnet  f ,  sufficiently  to  prevent  the  fork  b  from  maJ^g 
violent  vibrations.     The  zigzag  at  1  shews  this  branch. 

When  the  prongs  of  the  fork  move  apart,  the  conduction  will  be  broken  at  h, 
and  after  a  short  interval  again  completed  at  i,  so  that  the  current  now  passes 
from  g  through  the  lower  prong  of  the  fork  to  i,  and  thence  by  k  to  the  battery  at 
C).  But  at  the  moment  the  conduction  is  broken  either  at  h  or  at  i,  poweifdl 
secondary  currents  are  formed  by  induction  in  the  8  electro-magnets  of  the  tuning- 
forks,  which  would  emit  luminous  and  noisy  sparks  at  the  points  of  interruption, 
if  the  rush  of  electricity  were  not  partly  stored  for  the  moment  in  the  condenser  c, 
and  partly  discharged  through  the  very  great  resistance  dd. 

This  resistance  dd,  as  is  seen  by  the  figure,  forms  a  perpetual  connection  be- 
tween g  and  the  battery,  but  it  conducts  so  badly  that  no  sensible  part  of  the  cnr- 
.f  rent  can  pass  through  it,  except  at  the  moment  when,  on  breaking  the  conduction* 
the  great  electro-motive  force  of  the  secondary  currents  is  generated. 

The  arrangement  just  described  is  preferable  when  the  fork  in  front  of  the 
resonance  chamber  i  is  the  Octave  above  the  fork  b.  But  if  the  fork  opposite  i 
makes  the  same  number  of  vibrations  as  the  fork  b,  the  wire  i  k  must  be  removed, 
and  both  the  other  wires  ending  in  i  must  be  connected  with  h. 

To  exclude  particular  forks  from  the  circuit,  short  secondary  connections  of  the 
coils  of  wire  of  their  electro-magnets  are  introduced.  The  arrangement  is  shewn 
in  fig.  32,  p.  J 2 lb.  The  metal  knobs  h  h  are  connected  condnctively  with  the 
clamping  screw  g  in  which  the  wire  of  the  electro-magnet  terminates.  If  the 
lever  i  is  moved  down,  it  presses  with  some  friction  on  the  nearer  knob  h,  and 
forms  so  good  a  secondary  conducting  connection  for  the  wire  of  the  electro- 
magnets, tiiat  the  greater  part  of  the  electric  current  passes  by  h  h,  and  only 
an  infinitesimally  small  part  travels  round  by  the  much  longer  path  of  Uie  electro- 
magnets. 

As  regards  the  theory  of  the  motion  of  the  forks,  it  is  immediately  seen  that 
IT  the  force  of  the  current  in  the  electro-magnets  must  be  a  function  of  the  time. 
The  length  of  the  period  is  equal  to  the  period  of  a  vibration  of  the  interrupting 
fork  b.  Let  the  number  of  interruptions  in  a  second  be  n.  Then  the  strength  of 
the  current  in  the  electro-magnets,  and  consequently  the  magnitude  of  the  force 
exerted  by  the  electro-magnets  on  the  forks,  will  be  of  the  form : 

Aq-^Ai. cos  (2'jmt  +  Ci) -I- ila . cos {4imt  +  c^) 

4- ilj .  cos  (67m^ -f  c,)  +  &c. 

The  general  term  of  this  series  A^ .  cos  {iirmnt  •{-  c^)  is  adapted  for  setting  in 
motion  a  fork  making  mn  vibrations  in  a  second,  but  would  have  little  effect  on 
forks  otherwise  tuned. 


\  APPENDIX  IX. 

ON  THE  PHASES  OP  WAVES  CAUSED  BY  RESONANCE. 

(Seep.  1246.) 

Let  a  tuning-fork  be  brought  near  the  opening  of  a  resonance  chamber,  and  sup- 
pose the  ear  of  the  Ustener  to  be  at  a  great  distance  off  in  comparison  with  the 
dimensions  of  the  opening.  In  the  Journal  filr  reine  und  arwewandte  Mathe* 
matiky  vol.  Ivii.  pp.  1-72,  in  my  paper  on  the  *  Theory  of  Aerial  Vibrations  in  Tubes 
with  Open  Ends,'  I  have  shewn  that  if  a  sonorous  point  exists  at  jB  in  a  space  partly 
bounded  by  firm  walls,  and  partly  unbounded,  the  motion  of  sound  at  another 
point  Ay  in  the  same  space,  will  be  identical  in  intensity  and  phase  with  that 
which  would  have  existed  at  B  if  A  had  been  the  sonorous  point.  Let  B  be  the 
position  of  the  tuning-fork  (or  more  properly  of  the  end  of  one  of  its  prongs),  and 
A  that  of  the  ear.  The  motion  of  the  air  which  begins  when  the  tuning-fork  ia 
placed  near  the  opening  of  the  resonance  chamber,  is  not  easily  determined,  but 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


APP.tt.   PHASES  OP  WAVES  CAUSED  BY  EESONANCE.      401 

I  have  been  able  (in  pp.  47  and  48  of  the  paper  quoted  above)  to  determine  the 
motion  when  the  tuning-fork  is  at  a  great  distance.  Let  us  suppose,  then,  that  the 
fork  is  removed  to  the  position  of  the  ear  A,  and  we  shall  then  have  to  determine  the 
motion  of  the  sound  at  the  point  B  near  the  opening.  This  motion  is  composed 
of  two  parts  ;  the  first,  having  its  potential  denoted  by  ^  in  the  paper  cited,  corre- 
sponds with  the  motion  which  would  also  exist  if  the  opening  to  the  resonance 
chamber  were  closed,  and  in'the  above  case  is  too  small  to  be  sensible ;  the  second, 
there  marked  ^,  has,  in  open  space  and  at  some  distance  from  the  opening,  the 
following  value,  using  the  notations  explained  in  the  above  paper  (p.  28,  equation 
12  h), 

ir  =  -^.cos(A;^-27mi)    (i) 

2irp 

where  Q  is  the  sectional  area  of  the  resonance  tube,  p  the  distance  from  the  middle 

point  of  the  opening,  n  the  pitch  number,  ^  the  length  of  the  wave.    The  motion  IF 

at  an  infinitesimal  distance  r  from  the  sonorous  point  A  is  given  by  the  equation. 

^^^    cos(2irn^-c)     . 

r 

and  if  r^  be  the  distance  of  the  imaginary  sonorous  point  A  from  the  middle 

of  the  op^iing  of  the  resonance  tube,  we  find  from  equations  (i6c)  and  (13a)  of 

the  paper  cited : 

.      /,      ,    N      .  A;* .  Q .  sin  W .  cos  Ara  ,     • 

-tan  (A;ri+c)  =  tanr2  = ^ j--t- — -- (2a) 

^  27r .  cos  k\^L  +  a)  ^     ' 

(Z  length  of  tube,  and  a  a  constant  depending  on  tlie  form  of  its  opening),  and 
finally  by  the  same  equations,  the  magnitude  there  called  I  is : 

r     TT  2A: .  sin  A;Z  .    ^  X-.   Zf^ .  sin  ^Z 

Tx  2T .  sm  ra  H 

whence  it  follows  that  ^  =  ±H.4!;ii^5Ji    (3) 

The  si^  ±  is  to  be  so  determined  that  the  consonants  A  and  B.  have  the  same 
sign,  and  m  that  case  r^  must  lie  between  o  and  ir. 

Iq  this  case  the  strength  of  the  resonance  A  is  expressed  in  terms  of  the  in- 
tensity of  the  sonorous  point  H,  the  section  of  the  resonance  tube  Q,  the  distance 
Ti  of  the  sonorous  point  from  the  opening  of  the  tube,  and  the  magnitude  r,.  The 
difference  of  phase  between  the  points  A  and  B  is  shewn  by  equations  (i),  (2),  and 
(2a)  to  be 

TT — Zf/i  +  c  =  TT — A;p — fcri  —  r^. 

But  the  magnitude  Up  at  all  such  distances  of  the  point  B  firom  the  middle  of 
the  opening  as  we  can  use,  may  be  regarded  as  iofinitesimally  small,  so  that  when 
we  weaken  the  tone  by  withdrawing  the  fork  further  from  the  opening  of  the  tube,  f 
we  do  not  sensibly  change  the  phase  of  the  aerial  vibration.  But  if  we  change 
the  pitch  of  the  tube,  the  expression  for  the  phase  will  be  altered  only  through  a 
change  in  r^,  which  by  equation  (2a)  depends  on  hi,  and  to  this  change  there 
always  corresponds  a  change  in  the  strength  of  the  resonance,  since  sin  r^ 
appears  as  a  fEkctor  in  the  expression  for  that  resonance  in  equation  (3).  The 
resonance  is  strongest  when  sin  r,  =  i,  or  73  =  \v.  Calling  this  maximum  reson- 
ance il],  we  have 

and  for  other  pitches  of  the  tube,  supposing  its  sectional  area  Q  to  remain  un- 
changed, 

A 
sm  rj  =  -J-  . 

Whether  rj  is  to  be  taken  smaller  or  greater  than  a  right  angle,  depends  upon 

Digitized  by  \JtfDgie 


402      PHASES  OF  WAVES  CAUSED  BY  RESONANCE.   app.i^. 

whether  the  value  of  tan  r,  from  equation  (2a)  is  positive  or  negative.  Bat 
since  k,  Q  and  cos  ka  are  always  positive,  the  value  of  tan  r^  depends  on  the 

factor  — ?J5L The  maximum  resonance  corresponds  to  cos  Ar(Z  +  a)  =  o :  the 

cos  k(l'\- a)  ^  V  /  » 

minimum  to  sin  A;Z  =  o.  Hence  r^K^  when  bv  lengthening  the  tube  the  reson- 
ance is  brought  towards  its  minimum ;  but  T2>ffr  when  the  resonance  is  brougfat 
towards  its  maximum.  In  actual  application  the  tube  is  always  near  its  posi^on 
of  maximum  resonance,  and  hence  r2<^ir  when  the  tube  is  too  flat  in  pitch,  and 
r2>^7r  when  the  tube  is  too  sharp  in  pitch. 

If  we  put  the  tube  out  of  tune  to  such  an  extent  that  A*  =  ^.A^^,iihe  phase  of 
vibration  alters  by  ^.  Hence  we  are  always  able  to  estimate  the  amount  of  altera- 
tion of  phase  by  the  alteration  of  strength  of  resonance. 


x  =  - 


A  similar  law  holds  for  the  phases  of  the  vibrating  forks  as  compared  with 
^  those  of  the  exciting  current.  To  simplify  the  treatment,  I  will  consider  the  case 
of  a  single  vibrating  heavy  point,  which  is  constantly  restored  to  its  position  of 
rest  by  an  elastic  force.  When  the  heavy  point  is  moved  to  the  distance  x  from 
its  position  of  rest,  let— a^^  be  the  elastic  force.  Suppose  moreover  that  there 
act,  first  a  periodic  force,  similar  to  that  generated  by  the  electrical  currents  in  our 
experiments,  which  may  be  represented  by  il .  sin  nt,  and  secondly  a  force  which 
damps  the  vibrations  and  is  proportional  to  the  velocity,  so  that  we  may  write  it 

— ft*  -7>.    A  force  of  the  latter  kind  arises  in  our  experiments  partly  from  friction 
at 

and  resistance  of  the  air,  and  also  partly  from  the  currents  induced  by  the  tuning- 
forks  set  in  motion,  and  this  latter  part  has  most  effect  in  damping  the  vibrations. 
If  m  is  the  mass  of  the  heavy  vibrating  point,  we  have,  therefore, 

m.f^  =  -a»ir-6«^  +  i4.sinn^  (4) 

dt^  at 

^       The  complete  integral  of  this  equation  is 

^.J55J.sin(n^-f)  +  ^e"^.sin  ( -.>/(a«m-J6^) +c  j  ...    (4a) 

where  tan£=  +  -H =  (4b) 

a^  —  mri' 

The  term  having  the  coefficient  B  in  (4a)  is  sensible  only  at  the  beginning  of 

the  motion ;  on  account  of  the  factor  e    >»  it  decreases  with  the  increase  of  the 

time  tf  and  ultimately  vanishes.  But  its  existence  at  the  beginning  of  the  motion 
occasions  those  transient  beats  mentioned  in  App.  VIII.,  p.  3986,  when  n  is  slightly 
different  from 

I  >/  (a«m-i6«). 
m 

f       The  term  with  the  coefficient  A  in  equation  (4a),  on  the  other  hand,  corresponds 
to  the  sustained  vibration  of  the  heavy  point.     The  vis  viva  t*  of  this  motion  is 

equal  to  the  maximum  value  of  ^  m .  [  ~  ]  ,  or  to 

.,      m^«.8in»e  . 

2b*        ^^' 

When  the  pitch  of  the  exciting  tone,  that  is,  n,  can  be  altered,  i^  will  reach  its 
maximum  (which  we  will  call  P),  when 

8in*€=i,    dr    tan  €  =  ±00, 

givmg  ^'^Ib^' 

Hence  we  may  also  write 

••-. (5«) 

Digitized  by  VjOOQlC 


APt^.ix.      PHASES  OF  WAVES  CAUSED  BY  BESONANCE.  403 

The  same  magnitude  e  therefore  determines  in  equation  (4a)  the  difference 
of  the  phases  between  the  periodically  changing  displacements  x  of  the  heavy  point 
and  the  changing  values  of  the  force,  and,  in  equation  (5a),  the  strength  of  the 
resonance. 

The  condition  tan  £  =  ±  00  is  by  (4b)  falfilled  when  a*  «  m  n*. 

Hence  if  ^  be  the  value  of  n  which  answers  to  the  maximum  of  the  sym- 
pathetic vibration,  we  shall  have 


3 


J^  =  ^  (Sb) 

This  tone  of  strongest  resonance  is  the  same  as  the  tone  which  the  heavy 
point  would  occasion,  if  it  were  set  in  vibration  solely  by  the  influence  of  the 
elastic  force,  without  friction  and  without  external  excitement.  Somewhat,  differ- 
ent from  this  is  the  proper  tone  of  the  body,  which  it  produces  under  the  influence 
of  friction  and  resistance  of  the  air.  The  pitch  of  this  proper  tone  y  is  given  in  % 
the  second  term  of  the  equation  (4a) 

Not  until  6  =  0,  that  is,  not  until  the  friction  and  resistance  of  the  air  vanish, 

will  r^z^^^IP. 

m 

Now  in  all  practical  cases  where  we  have  to  observe  the  phenomenon  of 
sympathetic  vibration,  b  is  infinitesimal,  so  that  the  difference  between  the  tone 
of  greatest  resonance  and  the  proper  tone  of  the  vibrating  body  may  be  disregarded, 
as  in  the  text.    Introducing  the  magnitude  N  the  equation  (4b)  becomes 

*-'=^K^«) '^)    ^ 


On  account  of  the  question  raised  on  p.  150&  as  to  the  behaviour  of  the  basilar 
membrane  of  the  ear  for  noises,  we  are  interested  farther  in  the  integral  of  an  equa- 
tion in  which  il  sin  n^  of  equation  (4)  (p.  4026)  is  replaced  by  an  arbi^ary  function  of 
Uie  time  i/^^.  Of  course,  if  this  function  vanishes  for  very  great  positive  and  negative 
values  of  the  time,  it  could  be  transformed,  by  means  of  Fourier's  integral,  into  a 
Bum  (integral)  of  terms  such  as  il  sin  {nt  -f*  c),  and  then  for  each  one  of  these 
terms,  the  solution  just  found  might  be  applied,  and  finally  the  sum  of  all  these 
solutions  might  be  taken.  But  this  form  of  solution  becomes  mcomprehensible, 
because  it  eiddbits  a  continuous  series  of  tones  each  of  which  exists  from  ^ss— 00 
to  ^=  +  00 .    Hence  we  must  proceed  differently. 

The  differential  equation  to  be  integrated  is 

.-^+^'l+'»'-='^ (s)  , 

in  which  x  is  the  required,  and  \p  the  given  function  of  the  time,  1/^  being  assumed 
to  be  finite  for  all  values  of  t. 
Assume 

y  +  sA^A  r  tJ/,.e-^'-^.ds (6) 

where  k  represents  one  of  the  roots  of  the  equation 

mic«  +  &»ic  +  a»  =  o  (6a) 

that  IB 

«=_^±^f^-i*.)  (6b) 

whieh  we  will  represent  by 

Digitized  by  VAOOQlC 


404      .PHASES  OP  WAVES  CAUSED  BY  EESONANCE.   app.h. 


That  is,  X  is  shewn  to  be  a  sum  of  superposed  expiring  oscillations,  of  which  the 
initial  time  is  8,  and  the  initial  amplitude  -^^  and  every  moment  preceding  the 

point  of  time  t  contributes  to  the  result.  But  this  contribution  vanishes  for  those 
parts  of  the  motion  which  were  excited  for  a  long  time  before  the  moment  con- 
^  sidered,  that  is/for  those  for  which  the  exponent  a  {t—s)  is  a  large  number,  and 
the  motion  therefore  depends  at  every  instant  only  on  those  forces  i^  which  have 
acted  a  short  time  previously. 

If  the  action  of  the  force  \l/  takes  place  only  during  a  limited  time  from  to  to 
ti,  then  X  of  the  equation  (6d)  will  not  be=so  except  up  to  the  time  ^o,  lifter 
which  it  will  differ  from  nothing,  and  after  ti  the  motion  will  be  that  of  simply 
expiring  vibrations.  Also  the  magnitude  of  x  will  depend  upon  how  often  large 
positive  values  of  4/  occur  at  the  same  time  as  large  positive  values  of  sin  fit,  and 
negative  with  negative.  The  value  of  x  will  be  comparatively  the  greatest  when 
i//  and  sin  fit  change  their  sign  nearly  at  the  same  time. 

If  ;//(  has  had  a  constant  value  p  from  ^  =  ^0  to  ^3=^,,  then 


on  putting 


assuming  the  coefficient  of  damping  to  be  smaU  enough  for  the  root,  which  we  ' 
represent  by  fi,  to  be  possible. 

Hence,  if  1//  is  a  continuous  function,  j 

|^(y  +  rci)=A-P  J...e'('-\&  +  ^^, (6c) 

£^{y^xi)^AK^Vj..e<*-'\ds  +  AKx(^,+A^^  (6d) 

Then,  multiplying  (6)  by  a*,  (6c)  by  b^,  (6d)  by  m,  and  adding,  and  taking  account 
of  (6a),  we  obtain  the  following  equation  between  the  imaginary  parts  of  the  le- 
spective  expressions. 

Then  assuming  A  =  -— 

pm 

equation  (6)  gives  a  value  for  x  which  satisfies  the  di£Eerential  equation  in  8,  and 
is  finite  for  all  values  of  the  time,  namely 


X^He  -^'-^)  Bm{fi{t-ti)-he-ri] 
k  cos  ri  =  —  a,  fcsini/=)8 

-^  ^^  '  ""  ^  ^'  "^  '^'''^^  '''''  A^i -^0)] 

If  tire  suppose  k  to  have  the  positive  value  of  %/(♦«*  + j8*),  )?  wiU  be  an  obtuse 
angle.  If  we  give  H  the  sign  of  the  pressure  p,  then  the  angle  c,  which  hes 
between  +  ^^  and  —  '^,  will  have  the  same  sign  as  sin  fi{ti  -~  <o)*  ^  this  case  the 
expression  for  x  represents  expiring  vibrations,  of  which  the  initial  amplitude 
(patting  r  s  the  length  of  the  action  ss  t|  —  ^0)  has  the  value 

H^^JSL^s/  (1-2  c-'cos/Jr  +  e -»•'). 

This  is  a  maximum  for  different  values  of  r,  when  cos  (/3r  +  ij)  =5:  cos  ly .  c^*,  or, 
for  small  values  of  a  and  r,  when  fir  approximately  contains  an  uneven  number 

Digitized  by  V^OOQIC 


A^.  IX.  X.        SYMPATHETIC  RESONANCE.  405 

of  half  periods  of  vibration  of  the  proper  tone ;  and  on  the  other  hand  f  is  a 
minimmn  for  an  even  number  of  &uch  vibrations. 

After  long  continued  action  of  the  force  jp,  however,  the  exponential  functions 
vanish,  and  H  receives  the  constant  value 

H  ^    P 

On  the  other  hand,  for  very  small  values  of  r  the  initial  maxima  for  /3r  =  w,  may 
attain  the  value 

If  the  pressure  p  changes  its  sign  whenever  cos  j3r  does  so,  the  ampUtude  H  after 
n  such  changes  of  sign  will  be  ^ 

or 

H=  JL^ .  i±£Z  .  (I  ^  e-(»+i)-M. 

fimk     I  —  e  — '     ^  ^ 

This  expression  shews  the  reinforcement,  increasing  with  every  change  of  sign, 
which  ensues  upon  the  coincidence  of  the  period  of  change  of  pressure  with  the 
period  of  the  proper  tone.  The  denominator  (i  —  e  ~")  gives  the  amount  of  damp- 
ing during  half  the  periodic  time  of  vibration.  Finally,  when  this  is  very  smtJl 
H  will  be  very  large,  and  at  last,  afker  an  infinite  number  of  repetitions. 


I— e" 


APPENDIX  X. 

BSLATION  BETWEEN  THE   STRENGTH  OF  SYMPATHETIC  BEBONANCE  AND  THE 
LENGTH  OF  TIME  BEQITIBED  FOB  THE   TONE  TO  DIE  AWAY. 

(See  pp.  113a  and  142^2.) 

Retain  the  notation  of  App.  IX.,  for  the  motion  of  a  heavy  point,  reduced  to 
its  position  of  rest  by  an  elaistic  force.  When  such  a  point  is  agitated  by  an  ex- 
ternal periodic  force,  its  motion  is  given  by  equation  (4a),  p.  402c.  If  we  assume 
Af  the  intensity  of  this  force,  to  vanish,  equation  (4a)  reduces  to  f 

x^B.e     a»» •. sin  (fi  +  c) 
where  y=^  •  V  (a^w— i  6^). 

On  account  of  the  factor  which  contains  ^  in  the  exponent,  the  value  of  x 
continually  diminishes.  As  in  the  text,  measure  t  by  the  number  of  vibrations  of 
the  tone  of  strongest  resonance,  and  for  this  purpose  put 

2n 


/J      IT  6*        /IT    n\     ,  f^. 

/Vjoogle 


Digitized  by' 


4o6  VIBRATION  OF  THE  BASILAR  MEMBRANE.       app.  x.  n. 

Let  L  be  the  vis  viva  of  the  vibrations  at  the  time  tso,  and  I  at  the  time 
i^U  then 


.c-*^^ 


BO  that  l  =  e-»^^ 

Li 

and  T  =  iz.lognat~  (6a) 

In  the  table  on  p.  143a,  it  is  assumed  that  L^iol,  and  the  value  of  T  is  cal- 
culated on  this  assumption,  as  follows,  after  finding  the  value  of  p.  In  equation 
(6),  sin*  e  is  put  sss^,  corresponding  with  the  condition  that  the  strength  of  the 
%  tone  of  the  sympathetically  resonant  body  should  be  ^  of  the  maximum  strength 
it  can  attain ;  and  the  ratio  ^  :  n  is  calculated  from  the  numerical  ratios  corre- 
sponding to  the  intervals  mentioned  in  the  first  column  of  that  table. 
Equation  (4b)  in  App.  IX.,  p.  402c,  may  be  whttei^ 

V  p 

tan  £  = = 


„     fN     n\  fN     n\ 


In  this  equation  N,  giving  the  pitch  of  strongest  resonance ;  6',  the  strength 
of  the  friction;  and  m,  the  mass  may  be  different  for  various  fibres  of  Gorti. 
Hence  in  applications  to  the  ear,  we  must  consider  b^  and  m  to  be  functions  of  N. 
Now  since  the  degree  of  roughness  of  the  closer  dissonant  intervals  remains  tolerably 
constant  for  constant  intervals  throughout  the  scale,  the  magnitude  represented 

by  tan  t  muEt  assume  approximatively  the  same  values  for  equal  values  of  --,  and 
[  ** 

hence  the  magnitude  — ^=     niust  be  tolerably  independent  of  the  values  of  N. 

No  very  exact  result  can  be  obtained.     Hence  in  the  calculations  which  will 
follow  hereafter  p  is  assumed  to  be  independent  of  N. 


APPENDIX  XI. 

VIBBATION  OF  THE  MEMBRANA  BASXLABIB  IN  THE  OOOHLEA. 

(See  p.  1466.) 

f  The  mechanical  problem  here  attempted  is  to  examine  whether  a  connected  mem* 
brane  with  properties  similar  to  those  of  the  membrana  basilaris  in  the  cochlte, 
could  vibrate  as  Herr  Hensen  has  supposed  this  particular  membrane  to  do;  that 
is,  in  such  a  way  that  every  bundle  of  nerves  in  the  membrane  could  vibrate  sym- 
pathetically with  a  tone  corresponding  to  its  length  and  tension,  without  being 
sensibly  set  in  motion  b^  the  adjacent  fibres.  For  this  investigation  we  may  dis- 
regard the  spiral  expansion  of  the  basilar  membrane,  and  assume  it  to  be  stretched 
between  the  legs  of  an  angle,  of  the  magnitude  217.  Let  the  axis  of  x  bisect  this 
angle,  and  the  axis  of  ^  1^  drawn  at  right  angles  to  it  through  the  vertex  of  the 
angle.  Let  the  tension  of  the  membrane  parallel  to  the  axis  of  x  be  =  P,  and 
that  parallel  to  the  axis  of  ^  be  =  Q,  both  measured  by  the  forces  which  when 
exerted  on  the  sides  of  a  unit  square,  parallel  to  x  and  y  respectively,  would  balance 
the  tension  of  the  membrane.  Let  /j.  be  the  mass  of  this  unit  square,  t  the  time, 
and  z  the  displacement  of  a  point  in  the  membrane  from  its  position  of  equilibrium. 
Moreover  let  Z  be  an  external  force,  acting  on  the  membrane  in  the  direction  of 
positive  z,  and  setting  it  in  vibration.  The  equation  of  the  motion  of  the  mem- 
Digitized  by  V^jOOQIC 


Ai»r.  XI.  VIBRATION  OF  THE  BASILAR  MEMBRANE.  407 

brane,  deduced  without  material  difficulty  from  Hamilton's  principle  by  EirchhofTs 
process,  is  then 

^+^-®+«-i'='-4' • <'^ 

The  limiting  conditions  are 

1 )  that  2r  =  o  along  the  legs  of  the  angle,  that  is,  jsr  =  o,  when  y  =  ±  a? .  tan  >/. 

2)  that  £r  =  o,  when  a;  =  y  =  o,  that  is,  at  the  vertex  of  the  angle,  and  finally 

3)  that  z  is  finite,  when  x  is  infinite. 

The  farther  development  of  the  problem  will  shew  how  these  fcwo  last  limiting 
equations,  which  suffice  for  our  purpose,  may  be  replaced  by  certain  determinate 
curves  acting  as  fixed  boundaries  between  the  legs  of  the  angle  (p.  41  ic,  d). 

By  putting  x^i  ,  s/P  and  y  =1; .  n/  Q,  the  equation  (i)  may  be  reduced  to  the  ^ 
better  known  form 

which  is  the  equation  of  motion  for  a  membrane  stretched  uniformly  in  all  direc- 
tions, I  and  V  being  the  rectangular  co-ordinates  on  its  surface. 
For  this  notation  the  limiting  conditions  become 

i)  ;?=:o  for  i;=:±£  .  n/— .  tan  j;, 

2)  ;j=o  for  f  =  v  =  o, 

3)  z  finite,  for  5  =  00  . 

The  transformed  problem  consequently  differs  from  the  original  merely  in 
baying  a  uniformly  stretched  membrane,  and  a  different  amount  of  angle,  which 
we  will  represent  by  2e.  H 

Since  in  the  applications  which  we  have  to  make  of  the  result,  P  will  be  very 
small  in  comparison  with  Q,  the  angle  c  for  the  transformed  membrane  will  also 
be  very  small,  and  upon  this  circumstance  mainly  depend  the  analytical  difficulties 
of  the  problem. 

After  these  preliminary  remarks,  we  proceed  to  the  analytical  treatment  of  the 
equations  (i)  and  (la)  by  introducing  polar  co-ordinates,  assuming 

icssf  .  s/Pssr  .  n/P  .  cos  taf  1  i  y^ 

y  =  v  .  s/q^r  .  s/Q  .  sin  «/ ^'^^ 

The  equations  (i)  and  (la)  then  take  the  form 

d^z  .  1   dz  ,  1    d^z  d^z       „  ,-. 

dP+f  jr+p-5;?=''-5F-'^ ^"^ 

The  limiting  conditions  are  now,  that  ^ 

*  p 

i)  esso,  when  utss  +  c,  and  hence  tan  £=:^/-..  tan  17, 

2)  2r  =  o  for  r=o, 

3)  z  is  finite,  when  r  is  infinite. 

As  regards  the  nature  of  the  force  Z,  we  shall  assume  that  it  consists  of  two 

dz 
parts ;  the  first  depending  on  the  friction,  which  we  may  put  =  —  v  .  -,- ,  where  y 

is  a  positive  real  constant ;  the  second,  depending  on  a  periodically  variable  pres- 
sure exerted  by  the  surrounding  medium  on  the  membrane,  uniformly  over  its 
whole  surface.     Consequently  we  put 


at 


Digitized  by 


Google 


4o8  VIBRATION  OF  THE  BASILAB  MEMBRANE.  app.  xi. 

and  obtain  as  the  equation  of  motion 

£'+?-l+^-&='-»+-|-^-"- <»> 

Of  the  various  motions  which  the  membrane  could  execute  under  these  circam- 
stances,  we  are  interested  solely  in  those  which  are  maintained  by  the  continaons 
periodical  action  of  the  force,  and  which  must  themselves  have  itxe  same  period. 
Let  us  consequently  assume 

j?  =  f  .e'"*,  where  i=-v/(-i)   («») 

and  determine  C  by  the  equation 

t  ^K-fr*^-^^^---'"'-'—^  «"■' 

In  this  case  the  real  part  of  the  value  of  z  will  satisfy  equation  (2)  and  corre- 
spond to  a  uniformly  sustained  oscillation  of  the  membrane. 

Having  thus  eliminated  the  variable  t  from  the  differential  equation,  we  proceed 
to  do  the  same  for  w  by  means  of  the  first  limiting  equation,  after  transforming 
both  C  and  the  constant  A  into  a  series  of  cosines  of  uneven  multiples  of  the 

angle  —  =  ^.    It  is  well  known  that  between  the  limits  ^  s=s  +  ^  and  —  ^ 
2e 

il=:-^  .   /"cOS^— i.C08  3^  +  i.COSS;i«  +  ...}    (3) 

IT       V  3  5  ^ 

If  in  the  same  way  we  put 

4:=si .  cos  ^«  — i  .S3,  cos  3^+ '  •»6-oo8  s^+   (3ft) 

«'  35 

then  for  each  coefficient  8^  we  must  have 

And  since  the  first  of  our  limiting  conditions  is  satisfied  by  the  equation  (3a). 
whenever  the  series  converges,  there  remain  only  the  conditions  that 

i)  s,.  =  oforr=o, 
2)  s^  finite,  for  r  =  00 . 

It  is  easily  seen  that  every  s^  is  perfectly  determined  by  these  conditions.  For 
if  there  were  two  different  functions  which  satisfied  the  equation  (3b)  and  the  two 
limiting  conditions,  then  their  difference,  which  we  will  call  S,  would  satisfy  the 
conditions 

5^  +  r-5-r-^(''~-^^^-^-^^^    (^"^ 

and  hence  be  a  Bessel's  function,  and  at  the  same  time  we  should  have 

i)  3s=o  for  r  =  o, 
2)  2  finite  for  r  =  oo. 

But  these  two  conditions  cannot  be  satisfied  at  the  same  time  by  a  Bessel's  func- 
tion, when  V  has  a  value  differing  ever  so  little  from  o.  It  is  only  when  i^so, 
that  is,  when  there  is  no  friction,  that  the  determination  is  insufficient.  In  that 
case  oscillations  once  induced  may  continue  for  ever,  even  when  there  is  no  force 
to  give  fresh  impulses. 

Particular  integrals  of  the  equation  (3b)  may  be  easily  developed  in  the  form 
of  series,  resembling  the  series  for  the  related  BessePs  functions  which  satisfy 
equation  (3c).  One  of  these  series  proceeds  according  to  integral  powers  of  r  and 
is  always  convergent.    But  when  the  angle  c  is  very  small,  the  number  of  terms 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


APP.xi.  VIBRATION  OP  THE  BASILAR  MEMBRANE.  409 

in  this  series  which  are  necessary  to  determine  s  is  very  large,  and  hence  the  series 
ctmnot  be  used  for  determining  the  progress  of  the  function. — A  second  series 
which  proceeds  according  to  negative  powers  of  r  and  gives  a  second  particular 
integral  is  semi-convergent,  and  will  not  become  an  algebraical  function,  unless  h 
is  an  uneven  number.  But  in  the  latter  case  the  first  mentioned  series  will  be 
infinite  in  its  separate  terms. 

It  is  therefore  preferable  for  our  present  purpose  to  obtain  the  expression  for 
8  in  the  form  of  definite  integrals. 

Let  ^  and  }p  denote  the  following  pair  of  integrals : — 


^=1  u.e  \        uj 


du 


(4) 


where  ic=  v'lftn*— i»v) (4a)    % 

and  the  sign  of  the  root  is  so  chosen  that  the  possible  part  of  i  ic  is  positive* 
Then 

3^=:^.  (mA.i/'  +  m/t.^,cos  Jw&ir— i) (4b) 

which  is  the  required  expression  for  8^, 

To  shew  that  the  expression  in  (4b)  really  satisfies  the  equation  (3b),  substitute 
this  value  for  s^  in  that  equation,  and  in  differentiating  under  the  integral  signs  of 

1/^  and  ^,  use  partial  integration  to  eliminate  the  ia^tora  cos  t  and  [1^—-  ]  which 

appear  under  the  integral  signs. 
For  r  =  o  we  find 


f     du    ^    I 


mh 
and  hence  9,^  =  0. 

For  r=oo  ,  we  have  ^s=\/.s=:o,  and  hence 

8    — ^ 

Hence  the  function  5,,,  also  satisfies  the  two  limiting  conditions,  which  have 
been  already  shewn  to  be  sufficient  to  determine  it. 

The  equation  (4b)  may  be  used  to  determine  the  value  of  8^  when  P,  the  ten- 
sion of  the  membrane  in  the  direction  x,  is  infinitesimal.  Li  this  case,  as  (ib) 
shews,  r  must  be  the  iofinite ;  as  also  h,  of  which  the  value  is  -r 

ft-       ^>VQ 
2  .  v  P .  tan  ly* 
Hence  putting 

rzszhp 
p  will  be  the  finite,  namely 

2X .  tan  71 

It  is  easily  seen  that  under  these  circumstances  mhf  will  =  o.  For  we  may 
write 

mki,^r  mh.e-^f^'^ogu-il-i.)M.(u^iy^ ^^^ 

u 
where  I  have  put 

iics=i-iA^ 


Digitized  by 


Google 


4IO  VIBRATION  OF  THE  BASILAR  MEMBRANE.  app.  xl 

and  I  according  to  the  above  supposition  will  be  positive.  Since  within  the  whole 
extent  of  the  integration  t^  >  i  and  hence  log  u>o,  the  possible  part  of  the  ex< 
ponent  will  be  negative  throughout  the  same  extent,  and  will  contain  the  infinite 
factor  h.  Consequently  every  part  of  the  integral  vanishes,  and  hence  also  the 
whole  value  h  <f>. 

On  the  other  hand  the  integral  rp  or 


^-i:  e 


—  (Z— iX) .  ^.sin  ^ 

.  sin  mht .  dt 


will  have  the  possible  part  of  the  exponent  negative  and  infinite  for  all  those  parts 
of  the  integral  for  which  t  is  not  infinitesimal,  so  that  these  will  all  =  o.  Bat 
this  is  not  the  case  for  those  parts  of  the  integral  for  which  t  vanishes. 

Hence  for  an  infinite  h  we  may  replace  the  above  equations  for  ^  by  the 
f  following : 

^      Jo  .  emmht  .dt 

In  this  last  form  the  integration  may  be  effected  and  gives 

''"^  ^.[(Z-iA)«.f)«  +  m«]  J ^^*^ 

and  S^  = ,    a       a  a^ 

or,  by  (4a),  ^ 

,^  =  _^^i£.^.,-^j (5b) 

Or  if,  in  order  to  get  rid  of  the  auxiliary  magnitude  p,  we  represent  by  ^fi  the 
^  value  of  y  on  the  limits  of  membrane,  we  have 

ifi^x  tan  7/, 
and  hence  0  =  — '- — --, 

so  that  [using  S^  for  the  modulus  of  ^J,* 


S««=- 


4A 


!?^!![i^-».i»V4..«.i1     (5d) 


^[(^-A^n^)»  +  n».«] 


This  value  is  quite  independent  of  the  magnitude  of  the  angle  through  which 
the  membrane  is  stretched.  In  place  of  the  distance  p  or  a;  from  the  vertex,  we 
have  only  /3  the  breadth  of  the  membrane  at  the  point  in  question.  Hence  this 
expression  will  still  hold  when  the  angle  is  =  o,  and  the  membrane  vibrates  like  a 
string  between  two  parallel  lines,  thus  forming  m  vibrating  segments  which  are 
^  separated  by  lines  of  nodes  parallel  to  the  edges. 

The  same  expression  also  results  for  a  string,  if  j?  is  regarded  from  the  first  in 
equation  (i^  as  only  a  function  of  ^  in  a  line,  and  supposed  to  be  independent  of 
X,  but  the  limiting  condition  is  retained  that  when  ^=:  ±/3,  then  2r  =  o.  Hence 
the  motion  of  the  membrane  is  the  same  as  that  of  a  series  of  juxtaposed  but 
unconnected  strings.  

The  value  of  — .  5^  in  (sd)  gives  us  the  amplitude  of  the  corresponding  fonn 
m 

of  vibration  having  the  pitch  number  ^  ,  and  having  m  vibrating  transverse  divi- 

sions  of  the  membrane.    The  maximum  of  S^  will  occur  when 

m*ir«0-/3V»*  =  o (6) 

*  [In  the  3rd  German  edition  S^  is  used  <«  is  henceforth  nsed  for  S^ ;  consequentlj 
without  the  explanation  here  inserted;  in  the  the  reading  of  the  3rd  edition  has  been  re- 
4  th  German  edition  by  an  error  of  the  press      tained. — Translator,] 

Digitized  by  V^OOQIC 


APP.  XI.  VI.         THEORY  OF  COMBINATIONAL  TONES.  411 

The  valae  of  this  mftTimTiTn,  which  we  call  2„  is 

any 

The  smaller  the  coefficient  of  friction  v,  the  larger  ^nll  be  this  maximum  at  the 
point  in  question. 

If  we  cskH  h  the  value  of  /3  which  satisfies  the  equation  (6),  we  may  write  the 
equation  (sd)  thus 


/r      mU^Q^    f\     I  \n 


When  V  is  infinitesimal,  and  the  condition  of  the  maximum  is  not  fulfilled  in 
equation  (6),  the  denominator  of  this  expression  becomes  infinite,  and  hence  S^ 

infinitesimal.    The  amplitude  of  the  vibrations  1- .  £>,„,  will  become  finite,  for  If 

those  values  of  /3  only  which  are  so  nearly  =  &,  that  &  — /?  is  of  the  same  order 
as  V,  Under  these  circumstances,  therefore,  each  simple  tone  sets  in  vibration 
only  some  narrow  strips  of  the  membrane  in  the  direction  of  x,  of  which  the  first 

has  one,  the  second  three,  the  third  four,  &c.,  vibrating  segments,  and  in  which  ^, 

that  is  the  length  of  the  vibrating  segments,  has  always  the  same  value. 

The  greater  the  coefficient  of  friction  r,  the  greater  in  general  will  be  the 
extent  of  the  vibrations  of  every  tone  over  the  membrane. 

The  present  mathematical  analysis  shews  that  every  superinduced  tone  must 
also  excite  all  those  transverse  fibres  of  the  membrane  on  which  it  can  exist  as  a 
proper  tone  with  the  formation  of  nodes.  Hence  it  would  follow,  that  if  the 
membrane  of  the  labyrinth  were  of  completely  uniform  structure,  as  the  membrane 
here  assumed,  every  excitement  of  a  bundle  of  transverse  fibres  by  the  respective 
fundamental  tone  must  be  accompanied  by  weaker  excitements  of  the  unevenly  ^ 
numbered  harmonic  undertones,  the  intensity  of  which  would,  however,  be  mul- 
tiplied by  the  &ctors  i,  i- ,  and  generally  -L.  Although  this  hypothesis  has  been 
9  25  w* 

advanced  by  Dr.  Hugo  Biemann  in  his  Musikalische  Logik,  there  is  nothing  of  the 
kind  observable  in  the  ear.  I  think,  however,  that  this  cannot  necessarily  be  urged 
as  an  objection  against  the  present  theory,  because  the  appendages  of  the  basilar 
membrane  probably  greatly  impede  the  formation  of  tones  with  nodes. 


The  solution  can  also  be  extended  without  difficulty  to  the  case  where  the 
membrane  in  the  field  of  £,  v  is  bounded  by  two  circular  arcs,  with  their  centre  at 
the  vertex  of  the  angle.  To  this  case  correspond  as  boundaries  in  the  real  case, 
that  is,  in  the  field  of  x,  y,  two  elliptic  limiting  arcs,  which  when  P  vanishes  be- 
come straight  lines.  It  is  only  necessary  to  add  to  the  value  of  5,^  in  (4b),  a  com- 
plete integral  of  the  equation  (3c),  which  can  be  expressed  by  BessePs  functions 
with  two  arbitrary  constants,  and  to  determine  these  constants  in  such  a  manner  ^ 
as  to  make  «,»  =  o  on  the  limiting  curves  selected.  When  y  is  small  this  change 
in  the  limits  has  no  essential  efi'ect  on  the  motion  of  the  membrane,  except  when 
the  maximum  of  vibration  itself  falls  in  the  neighbourhood  of  the  limiting  curves. 


APPENDIX  Xn. 

THEOBY  OF  COMBIKATIONAIi  TONES. 

(See  pp.  152,  note  f,  and  158,  note  *.) 


It  is  well  known  that  the  princ^)le  of  the  undisturbed  superposition  of  oscillatory 
motions,  holds  only  on  the  supposition  that  the  motions  are  small, — so  small, 
indeed,  that  the  moving  forces  excited  by  the  mutual  displacements  of  the  par- 
ticles of  the  oscillating  medium  should  be  sensibly  proportional  to  these  displace- 

Digitized  by^OOQlC 


412  THEORY  OP  COMBINATIONAL  TONES.  apf.mt. 

ments.  Now  it  may  be  shewn  that  combinational  tones  must  arise  whenever  the 
vibrations  are  so  large  that  the  square  of  the  disjplacements  has  a  sensible  in^ 
fiuence  on  the  m>otions.  It  will  suffice  for  the  present  to  select,  as  the  simplest 
example,  the  motion  of  a  single  heavj  point  onder  the  influence  of  a  system  of 
waves,  and  develop  the  corresponding  result.  The  motions  of  the  air  and  other 
elastic  media  may  be  treated  in  a  pei^ectly  similar  manner. 

Suppose  that  a  heavy  point  having  the  mass  m  is  able  to  oscillate  in  the 
direction  of  the  axis  of  x.  And  let  tiie  force  which  restores  it  to  its  position  of 
equilibrium  be 

isKoaj  +  ia?*- 

Suppose  two  systems  of  sonorous  waves  to  act  upon  it,  with  the  respective 
forces 

/ .  sin ptf    and    g  .em  (qt  +  c) 

%  then  its  equation  of  motion  is 

cPx 
—m.  -^  ss  ax  -^  bx^  -hf .  Bin  pt  +  g  .  Bm  {qt  -^  c)» 

This  equation  may  be  integrated  by  a  series,  putting 

and  then  equating  the  terms  multiplied  by  like  powers  of  e,  separately  to  zero. 
This  gives 

1)  axi+m.-j^  =  -fi.anpt-gi  .em(qt  +  c), 

af 
3)  ax^+  m .  -7^  =  —  2bxi ^8»  ^^^  so  on. 

From  the  first  equation  we  obtain 

Xi^A  .  sin^i  .  N/^-+fc  j  +  t* .  sin^i  +  v  .  Bin  (g^  +  c) 

where  tt=s  — /l —    and    v  =  — ^ — . 

mpr^a  mq^—a 

This  is  the  well-known  result  for  infinitesimal  vibrations,  shewing  that  the 

body  which  vibrates  sympathetically  produces  only  its  proper  tone  >/^,  together 

%  with  those  communicated  to  it,  p  and  q.    Since  the  proper  tone  in  this  case 
rapidly  disappears,  we  may  put  Jl  a=o.    And  then  equation  (2)  gives 

2a  ' 


2(4mp' 


^ .COS2pt -, ^ 1  .  COS  2(qt  +  c) 


^^  .  cos  [(i,  -  q)t^c]  -._--^^^^_  .  COS  [(;,  +  q)t  +c]. 


The  second  term  of  the  series  for  x  [involving  x^,  contains,  then,  a  constant, 
and  also  the  tones  2p,  2q,  (^—9)9  and  (j>  +  ^)»    If  the  proper  tone  n/^  of  the 

body  which  vibrates  sympathetically  is  deeper  than  (2?  — g)»  aa  mav  be  certainly 
assumed  in  most  cases  lor  the  drumskin  of  the  ear  in  connection  with  the  auditory 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


App.xn.  xm.    MECHANISM  OF  THE  POLYPHONIC  SIREN.  413 

ossicles,  and  if  the  intensities  u  and  v  are  nearly  the  same,  the  tone  {p  —  q)  -will 
have  the  greatest  intensity  of  all  the  tones  in  the  terms  of  x^ ;  it  corresponds  with 
the  weU-biown  deep  combinational  tone.  The  tone  (p  +  q)  will  be  much  weaker, 
and  the  tones  2p  and  2q  will  be  heard  with  difficulty  as  weak  harmonic  upper 
partial  tones  of  me  generating  tones. 

The  third  term  x^  [of  the  series  for  x]  contains  the  tones  ^p,  $q,  2^+?,  2^—9, 
p-h2q,p^2q^p  and  q.  Of  these  2p^q  or  2q^p  is  a  combinational  tone  of  the 
second  order  according  to  Hallstroem's  nomenclature  (p.  154^).  Similarly  the 
fourth  term  x^  [of  the  series  for  x]  gives  combinational  tones  of  the  third  order ; 
and  so  on. 

If,  then,  we  assume  that  in  the  vibrations  of  the  tympanic  membrane  and  its 
appendages,  the  sq^uare  of  the  displacements  has  an  effect  on  the  vibrations,  the 
preceding  mechamcal  developments  give  a  complete  explanation  of  the  origin  of 
combinational  tones.  Thus  the  present  new  theory  explains  the  origin  of  the 
tones  (^  -fg),  as  well  as  of  the  tones  {p^q)t  and  shews  us,  why  when  the  intensities 
t^  and  V  of  the  generating  tones  increase,  the  intensity  of  the  combinational  tones,  «- 
which  is  proporaonal  to  uv,  increases  in  a  more  rapid  ratio. 

The  previous  assumption  respecting  the  magnitude  of  the  force  called  into 
action,  namely 

k=:ax  +  boc^ 

implies  that  when  x  changes  its  sign,  k  changes  not  merely  its  sign,  but  also  its 
absolute  value.  Hence  this  assumption  can  hold  only  for  an  elastic  body  which  is 
nnsymmetrically  related  to  positive  and  negative  displacements.  It  is  oiUy  in  such 
that  tiiQ  square  of  the  displacement  can  affect  the  motion,  and  combinational 
tones  of  the  first  order  arise.  Now  among  the  vibrating  parts  of  the  human  ear, 
the  drumsldn  is  especially  distinguished  by  its  want  of  symmetry,  because  it  is 
iorcibly  bent  inwards  to  a  considerable  extent  by  the  handle  of  the  hammer,  and  I 
venture  therefore  to  conjecture  that  this  peculiar  form  of  the  tympanic  membrane 
conditions  the  generation  of  combinational  tones. 

[See  especially  App.  XX.  sect.  L.  art.  5.]  ^ 


APPENDIX  Xm. 

DBSOSIPTIOK  OF  THE  IfEOHANIBM  EMPtiOTED  FOB  OPENING  THE  SEVERAL  SERIES 
OF  HOLES  IN   THE   POLYPHONIC   SIREN. 

{See  p.  162,  note  *.) 

Fia.  65  (p.  414a,  b)  shews  the  vertical  section  of  the  upper  box  of  the  double  siren,  in 
order  to  display  its  internal  construction.  E  is  the  wind  pipe  which  is  prolonged 
into  the  interior  of  the  box,  and  firmly  fixed  in  the  cross  beam  AA  of  the  sup- 
port of  the  apparatus.  The  prolongation  of  the  wind  pipe  into  the  box  B  has 
conical  surfaces  at  its  upper  and  lower  ends,  on  which  slide  corresponding  hollow  tr 
surfaces  in  the  bottom  and  top  surfaces  of  the  box,  so  that  this  box  can  revolve 
freely  about  the  wind  pipe  as  an  axis.  At  a  may  be  seen  a  section  of  the  toothed 
wheel  fastened  to  the  cover  of  the  box.  At  /3  is  the  driving  wheel  which  is  turned 
by  the  handle  y ;  and  2  is  a  pointer  which  is  directed  to  the  graduation  on  the 
edge  of  the  disc  te. 

D  is  the  upper  extremity  of  the  axis  of  the  movable  discs,  of  which  only  the 
npper  one  CC  is  here  shewn.  The  axis  turns  on  fine  points  in  conical  cups.  The 
upper  cup  is  introduced  into  the  lower  end  of  the  screw  17,  which  can  be  more  or 
less  tightened  by  the  milled  screw  head  introduced  above,  so  that  any  required 
degree  of  ease  and  steadiness  in  the  motion  of  the  axis  may  be  attained. 

Inside  the  box  are  seen  the  sections  of  four  pierced  rings  k\,  X/i,  fiv,  and  vo, 
which  fit  on  to  one  another  with  oblique,  tile-shaped  edges,  and  thus  mutually  hold 
each  other  steady.  Each  of  these  rings  lies  beneath  a  series  of  holes  in  the  cover, 
and  contains  precisely  the  same  number  of  holes  as  the  corresponding  series  of  the 
cover  and  of  the  rotating  disc.    By  means  of  studs,  shewn  at  ii  in  fig.  56  (p.  162), 

Digitized  by  V^OOQIC 


414 


VARIATION  OP  PITCH  IN  BEATS. 


App.  xin.  xrv. 


these  four  rings  can  be  slightly  displaced,  so  as  either  to  make  the  holes  of  the 
ring  coincide  with  the  holes  of  the  box,  and  thus  give  &ee  passage  to  the  air  and 
produce  the  corresponding  tone ;  or  else  to  close  the  holes  of  the  cover  by  the 


interspaces  between  the  holes  of  the  ring,  and  thus  cut  off  their  corresponding 
tone  idtogether. 

In  this  way  it  is  possible  to  sound  the  various  tones  of  this  siren  in  succession 
or  simultaneously,  and  hence  obtain  separate  or  combined  tones  at  pleasure. 


APPENDIX  XIV. 

VABUTION   IN  THE   PITCH  OF  SIMPLE  TONUS  THAT  BEAT   TOGETHEB. 

(See  p.  1656  and  note  *.) 

Let  t;  be  the  velocity  of  a  particle,  which  vibrates  under  the  influence  of  two  tones, 
so  that 

V  =A  .  Bm  mt  +  B  .  an  (nt  +  c) 

where  m  differs  very  slightly  from  w,  and  A>B.    We  may  then  put 

n^  +  c  =  m^  —  [[m  ^n)t^c\,  and 

^        f)=  [A  +3  .  cos  [(m  —  n)  t^c]]  .  Bmmt—B  .  sin  [(w— n)  *  — c]  .  eosm^ 

Assume 

A-\ 

B  .  sm  I 


and 
80  that 


A+B  ,QOB  [im  —  n)  ^  —  c]  =s:  C  .  cos  €, 


sm  £, 


t?  =  C  .  sin  {mt — c), 


in  which  C  and  e  are  functions  of  the  time  f,  which  will  alter  slowly,  if,  as  we  have 
assumed,  m  —  n  is  small  in  comparison  with  m. 

The  intensity  (?  of  this  osciUation  is  determined  by 

C^^A^+2  AB  .QOB  [(m-w)  *-c]  +  5«, 

and  it  will  be  a  maximum, 

C=  {A  +  J5)*,    when  cos  [(m  -  n)  *  -  c]  =  +  x, 

Digitized  by  V^OOQlC 


App.  XIV.  XV.  INTENSITY  OP  BEATS  OF  DIPFEBENT  INTERVALS.  415 

and  a  minimum, 

C^=(il— B)*,    whencos  [(m  — w)  ^  — c]  =  — I, 

The  variable  phase  e  of  the  motion  is  determined  by 

tanc-      ^.Bin[(m-n)^-c] 
A'\-B  .COS  [{m  —  n)  t  —  c] 

A.B  A>B,  this  tangent  never  becomes  infinite,  and  hence  e  remains  included 
between  the  limits  +^  and  —  ^ir,  to  which  it  alternately  approaches.  As  long 
as  e  increases,  mt  —  e  mcreases  more  slowly  than  mt ;  as  long  as  c  diminishes, 
mt  —  €  increases  faster  than  nU ;  hence  in  the  first  case  the  tone  flattens  and  in 
the  second  it  sharpens. 

The  pitch  number  of  the  variable  tone,  multiplied  by  2  tt,  is  under  these  cir- 
cumstances equal  to  1 

_  Jg ^  m i4'+  {m-^-n)  ,  AB  .  COB  [(m  —  w)  t  — c]  +  n  jg' 
dt         il«+  2  .ili?.  cos  [(m-n)^-c]  +    B^ 

The  limits  for  the  pitch  number  therefore  correspond 

to  cos  [(m  —  n)t  —  c]  becoming  +  i  or  —  i, 

and  hence  also  to  a  maximum  or  minimum  strength  of  tone. 

i)  When  the  strength  of  tone  is  a  maximum,  the  pitch  number  varies  as 

mA  +nB  __^_{m  —  n)  B  __^  ^(m'-n)  A 
A+B    "^     TTB        ""^     A+B    ' 

3)  When  the  strength  of  tone  is  a  minimuTn,  the  pitch  number  varies  as 

mA  —nB_^.  (m—  n)  B     ^  .im  —  n)  A  f 

-Zirfi-='*+ ^-iri- =»+ -JITS- 

Hence  in  the  first  case,  [or  during  the  maximum  strength,]  the  pitch  of  the 
variable  tone  Ues  between  the  pitches  of  the  two  separate  tones.  But  during  the 
minimum  strength,  if  the  stronger  tone  is  also  the  sharper,  the  pitch  of  the 
variable  tone  is  sharper  than  that  of  either  of  the  single  tones;  and  if  the  stronger 
tone  is  the  flatter,  the  pitch  of  the  variable  tone  is  flatter  than  that  of  either  of 
the  single  tones. 

These  differences  are  well  heard  with  two  stopped  pipes ;  and  also  with  two 
tuning-forks  when  first  the  higher  and  then  the  lower  is  placed  nearer  to  the 
resonance  chamber. 

[See  Mr.  Sedley  Taylor's  paper  on  this  subject,  Philosophical  Maaazine^  July 
1872,  pp.  56-64,  where  he  gives  several  figures  illustrating  the  variabiUty  of  the 
pitch,  and  deduces  the  above  results  (i)  and  (2)  firom  the  figures  only.] 


APPENDIX  XV. 

CALCULATION  OF  THE  INTENSITY  OF  THE  BEATS  OF  DIFFEBENT  INTEBVALS. 

(See  pp.  xSya  and  note*  and  193,  note  *.) 

We  shall  again  employ  the  formulae  for  the  strength  of  the  sympathetic  vibration 
developed  in  Appendix  IX.,  equations  (4a)  and  (4b),  p.  402c,  and  (5)  and  (5a), 
p.  402S.  For  the  tone  of  strongest  resonance  in  one  of  Corti's  elementary  organs, 
let  n  be  its  number  of  vibrations  in  2ir  seconds,  n^  and  n^  the  corresponding  num- 
bers of  vibrations  in  2ir  seconds  for  two  tones  heard,  and  B,  B"  the  greatest 
velocities  of  the  vibrations  which  they  superinduce  in  those  Corti's  organs  which 
have  the  same  pitch,  and  B^^  B^  the  greatest  velocities  which  both  attain  in  their 

Digitized  by  V^OOQIC 


4i6     INTENSITY  OP  BEATS  OP  DIFFEBENT  INTEBVALS.    app.xv. 

representation  of  the  number  of  vibrations  n.  Then  by  equation  (5a)  of  Appen- 
dix IX.,  p.  402d,  we  have 

Bi  =B'  sin  ei,    and   B^^B'^ .  sin  e, 

where  «• .  tan  «,  =  — — — ,    and    «• .  tan  t^  = — 1- — , 

ni      n  n^      n 

and  /3  is  a  magnitude  which  may  be  regarded  as  independent  of  n.  Hence  the 
intensity  of  the  vibrations  of  the  organ  for  the  number  of  vibrations  n,  when  both 
tones  Uy  and  n^  affect  it  simultaneously,  fluctuates  between  the  values 

{B^+B^f   and    (B^^B^)\ 

The  difference  of  these  two  magnitudes,  which  measures  the  strength  of  the 
f  beats,  is 

4B,  -82  =  4  B'  B"  .  sin  e,  .  sin  «,    (7) 

Hence  for  equal  differences  in  the  amount  of  pitch,  the  strength  of  the  beats  is 
dependent  on  the  product  B'  B".    For  the  mth  partial  tone  of  the  compound  tone 

of  a  violin,  we  may,  by  Appendix  VI.,  p.  597c,  put  5'*  =  — ^ ,  and  hence  \1  the  With 

rn, 

and  m^th  partial  tone  of  two  compound  tones  of  a  violin,  beat,  we  may  put  the 

intensity  of  their  beats  for  equal  differences  of  interval  = . 

m\  .  m^ 

This  is  the  expression  &om  which  the  numbers  in  the  last  column  of  the  table 
on  p.  187&  have  been  calculated.  [They  are  therefore  100  times  the  reciprocak  of 
the  products  of  the  two  numbers  which  give  the  ratio  of  the  pitch  numbers  in  the 
corresponding  line  of  its  third  column.] 

For  tiie  calculation  of  the  degree  of  roughness  of  the  various' intervals,  men- 
tioned in  pp.  193,  332,  and  333,  the  following  abbreviations  of  notation  are  in 
1[  troduced : 

w,  +  n,  =  2  J»f. 
n,  =JV(i+5). 

n^  =iV  (i  - 1). 

n  =^(i  +  >')- 

So  that 

V  .  tan  ei= — ; — - — —5    8^d   V  .  tan  £5=- 


I  -^  S     1  -j-  y  I  —  oi-f-r 

Since  powerful  sympathetic  resonance  ensues  only  when  v  and  S  are  very  small, 
we  may  assume  that,  approximatively, 

tan  c,=  — -1- .,    and    tan  €•= 1 . 

27r(»'— d)  23r(i'-f-<)) 

U        Putting  these  values  for  cj  and  f2>  ^  equation  (7)  we  have 

4  5.  B,=  4 B'  B"  .  ^^^  ^^,  ^  ^^ _  ^^^  '^ ^_^  ^^,    ^^ _^  ^^,^   ...     (7a) 

If  then  we  consider  y,  that  is,  the  pitch  of  the  Gorti's  organ  which  vibrates 
sympathetically,  to  be  variable,  4  B^  B^  will  reach  its  maximum  when  v==^,  and 
hence  n=iVs=^  [n^  +  n^),  and  if  we  call  the  value  of  this  maximum  s  we  have 

'-'^'^'-w^ ('»') 

In  calculating  the  degree  of  roughness  arising  from  sounding  two  tones  together 
which  differ  &om  each  other  by  the  interval  2I,  I  have  thought  it  sufficient  to 
consider  this  maximum  value,  which  exists  in  those  Corti's  organs  which  are  most 
favourably  situated.     Undoubtedly  other  beats  of  a  weaker  kind  will  be  excited  in 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


APP.  XV.     INTENSITY  OF  BEATS  OF  DIFFERENT  INTERVALS.     417 

the  neighbouring  fibres,  but  their  intensity  rapidly  diminishes.  It  might  therefore 
appear  to  be  a  more  exact  process  to  integrate  the  value  of  4  5,  J?,  in  equation 
(7a]  with  respect  to  r,  in  order  to  obtain  the  sum  of  the  beats  in  all  Corti^s  organs. 
This  would  require  an  at  least  approximate  knowledge  of  the  density  of  Corti's 
organs  for  different  values  of  y,  that  is,  for  different  parts  of  the  scale,  and  of  that 
we  know  nothing.  In  sensation,  the  highest  degree  of  roughness  is  certainly  more 
Important  than  the  distribution  of  a  less  degree  of  roughness  over  many  sensitive 
organs.  Hence  I  have  preferred  to  take  only  the  maximum  of  the  vibrations  from 
(7b)  into  account. 

Finally  we  have  to  consider  that  very  slow  beats  cause  no  roughness,  and  that 
•when  the  intensity  of  the  beats  remains  unaltered,  and  their  number  increases, 
the  roughness  reaches  a  maximum  and  then  decreases.  To  express  this,  the  value 
of  s  must  be  also  multiplied  by  a  factor,  which  vanishes  when  the  number  of  beats 
is  small,  attains  a  maximum  for  about  30  beats  in  a  second,  and  then  diminishes, 
and  again  vanishes  when  the  number  of  such  beats  is  infinite.  Suppose  then  that 
the  roughness  r^,  due  to  the  j?th  partial  tone,  is  expressed  by  ^ 

4eva^£f 

The  factor  of  Sp  reaches  its  maximum  value  =s  i,  when  p^^0\  and  becomes 
=:  o,  when  0,  that  is,  half  the  interval  between  the  two  tones  in  the  scale,  is  =  o 
or  00 .  Since  h  may  be  indifferently  positive  or  negative,  the  expression  can  only 
involve  even  powers  of  h.  The  above  is  the  simplest  expression  which  satisfies 
the  conditions,  but  it  is  of  course  to  a  certain  extent  arbitrary. 

For  0  we  must  put  half  the  extent  of  the  interval  which  at  the  pitch  of  the 
lower  beating  tone  causes  30  beats  to  be  made  in  a  second. 

Since  we  have  taken  &  with  264  vibrations  in  a  second,  as  the  lower  tone,  0 

has  been  put  =  -^ .    Hence  we  have  finally 
264 

r  =  16  B'B-   ^^'cy^_ 

•(/3^+4'r«^»)(«Hi^«t»r  % 

And  from  this  formula  I  have  calculated  the  roughness  of  the  intervals,  shewn 
graphically  in  the  diagrams,  fig.  60,  A  and  B,  p.  193^,  c,  and  fig.  61,  p.  333a. 
The  roughnesses  due  to  the  separate  partial  tones  have  been  drawn  separately 
and  superimposed  on  one  another  in  the  drawing. 

Although  the  theory  leaves  much  to  be  desired  in  the  matter  of  exactness,  it  at 
least  serves  to  shew  that  the  theoretical  view  we  have  proposed  is  really  capable 
of  explaining  such  a  distribution  of  dissonances  and  consonances  as  actually  occurs 
in  nature. 

Professor  Alfred  M.  Mayer,  of  Hoboken,  New  Jersey,*  has  instituted  experi- 
ments on  the  duration  of  sensations  of  sound,  and  the  number  of  audible  beats. 
Between  a  vibrating  tuning-fork  and  its  resonator  he  interposed  a  revolving  disc 
witii  openings  of  the  same  shape  as  that  of  the  resonator,  so  that  the  sound  was 
heard  loudly  when  an  opening  in  the  disc  came  in  front  of  that  in  the  resonator, 
and  faintly  when  the  latter  was  coshered.  His  results  agree  essentially  with  the 
assumptions  I  have  made  on  pp.  143  to  145,  and  183  to  185,  but  are  more  com-  m 
plete  as  they  have  been  pursued  throughout  the  whole  scale.  His  conclusions  are 
as  follows : — 

*  SiUiman's    Journal,  ser.    iil.  vol.   viii.  tinaity  wonld  have  the  ratio  2048  :  2249,  giving 

October   1874,  PhUoaqphicdl  Moffazine,  May  162  cents,  and  the  interval  of  maximum  disso- 

1875,  ^<>1*  ii*    [From  the  following  table,  p.  nance  would  be  67  cents,  and  hence  the  beats 

418a,  it  is  seen  that  the  interval  of  a  minor  of  b'"  c""  should  be  quite  conspicuous,  agree- 

Third  as  the  limit  of  continmty  applies  only  ing  with  observations.  In  reference  to  p.  1446, 

to  the  Octave  c  to  c'.    For  gf"",  supposing  the  Prof.  Mayer  observes  that  the  law  abruptly 

law  connecting  D  and  N,  given  on  p.  4i&i,  to  breaks    down    for  vibrations  below  40  in  a 

hold  for  such  a  high  pitch,  the  interval  of  con-  second,  and  thinks  that  this  abrupt  breaking 

tinaity  would  have  the  ratio  3072  :  3072  +  225,  down   *  can  only  be  explained  by  the  highly 

or  122  cents,  and  the  interval  of  maximum  probable  supposition  that  co-vibrating  bodies 

dissonance  would  be  49  cents,  so  that  the  in  the  ear,  tuned  to  vibrations  below  40  per 

interval  of  one  Semitone  is  near  the  limit  of  second,  do  not  exist,  and  therefore  ...  the 

continnity ;  hence  it  is  not  surprising  that  no  inner  ear  .  .  .  can  only  vibrate  en  masse,*  and 

beats  were  heard  in  the  ease  referred  to  on  also  that  such  oscillations  cannot  last  ^  sec. 

p.  1 73c,  note  *.    But  for  c""  the  interval  of  con-  —  Translator,] 

Digitized  by  ^JOOgle 


4i8 


BEATS  OP  COMBINATIONAL  TONES. 


APP.  XV.  XTl. 


Number  of  the 

besto  for  which 

the  interruptions 

ranish 

[Cents  in  the 

Number  of  beats 

[Gents  in  the 

Note 

Pitch  number 

corresponding 
interral* 

lor  the  greatest 

interrmlf 

c 

64 

16 

386 

6-4 

165           ! 

c 

128 

26 

308 

104 

135           1 

d 

256 

47 

292 

188 

123 

^^ 

384 

60 

251 

240 

105 

c" 

512 

78 

245 

31-2 

loa 

e" 

640 

90 

228 

36-0 

9S 

9" 

768 

109 

230 

43-6 

§5 

c"' 

1024 

US 

214] 

540 

89] 

APPENDIX  XVI. 

ON   BEATS  OF  COMBINATIONAL  TONES,   AND  ON  COMBINATIONAL  TONES   IN   THE 

SIREN  AND   HABMONIUM. 

(See  pp.  199a  and  note  *,  also  155c  to  158a.) 

Let  a,  h,  c,  d,  e,/,  g,  h  be  whole  numbers.  Let  an  and  ^n+^  be  the  pitch  nomben 
of  the  primes  of  two  compound  tones  sounded  simultaneously,  where  B  is  supposed 
to  be  very  small  in  comparison  with  n,  and  a  and  b  are  the  smallest  whole  numbers 
by  which  the  ratio  a  :  b  can  be  expressed.  The  pitch  numbers  of  any  pair  of 
partial  tones  of  these  two  compound  tones  will  be 

acn  and  bdn-^-dd. 
These  will  beat  with  each  other  dh  times  in  a  second,  if 

1^  ac^bd    or    r  =  ~' 


And  since  the  ratio  •-  is  expressed  in  its  lowest  terms,  the  smallest  values  of 
o 


d  and  c  are 

and  their  other  values  are 


d^a   and   c=s&, 
d^ha    and   c  =  %2>. 


Now  0  and  d  represent  the  ordinal  numbers  of  the  partial  tones  which  beat 
together.  Hence  the  lowest  partials  of  this  kind  will  be  the  bih  partial  of  tiie 
compound  a  n,  and  the  ath  partial  of  the  compound  bn+L  The  resulting  number 
of  beats  is  a  3. 

In  the  same  way  the  25th  partial  of  the  first  compound,  and  the  2ath  of  the 
%  second  give  2a  c  beats,  and  so  on. 

The  first  differential  combinational  tone  of  the  two  partials  acn  and  bdn  +  di 
is 

±l{bd'^ac)n  +  dh] 

where  the  +  or  *  sign  has  to  be  taken  so  that  the  whole  expression  is  positive. 


*  [The  interval  is  foand  as  the  ratio  of  the 
pitch  number  to  the  same  increased  by  the 
nomber  in  the  next  colomn  to  it;  thus  for 
C  it  is  64  :  64+  i6»4  :  5,  and  for  g'  it  is 
384:384  +  60=96:111,  and  from  these  I 
have  calculated  the  cents  as  in  p.  701  of  the 
ist  edition. 

If^l^  be  the  pitch  number,  and  I>»  dura- 
tion of  residual  sensations  or  Uie  reciprocal  of 
the  numbers  of  vibrations  producing  a  con- 
tinuous sound,  16,  26,  (tc,  as  in  the  next  pre- 


ceding column,  then  Prof.  Mayer  finds— 


.4). 


seconds.  —  Tratu- 


^^U  +  23  + 

lator,^ 

f  [The  interval  is  found  as  the  ratio  of  the 
pitch  number  to  the  same  increased  by  the  last 
mentioned  number  of  beats,  thus  64  :  644-6-4. 
Prof.  Mayer  draws  attention  to  the  fact  that 
his  beats  were  all  tones  of  the  same  pitch, 
whereas  the  beats  of  imperfect  consonances 
are  tones  of  variable  pitch.--7Vafu2aior.] 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


App.  XVI.  COMBINATIONAL  TONES  OP  SIREN,  ETC.  4^9 

Two  other  partials/aTi  and  gbn-^-  g^  give  the  differential  combinational  tone 

±[(gb-af)n'^gh]. 
When  both  sound  together  they  produce  (gzfd)^  beats,  if 
6eZ  — ac=±(^6  — a/) 
a     gz^d 


or 


b-fzfc 


As  before,  it  follows  that  the  least  value  of  ^  +  eZ  is  =  a,  and  the  other  (greater) 
values  are  =  ^a,  so  that  the  smallest  number  of  beats  is  a^. 

To  find  the  lowest  values  of  the  partials  which  must  be  present  in  order  to 
beat  with  the  first  differential  tones,  we  will  take  the  lower  signs  for  c  and  d,  and 
we  thus  obtain : 

gr  =  eZ  =  ia,  or  ^  =  i  (a  +  i),  and  d  =  i 
/=:c  =  |fc,  or/  =  |(6  +  i),  and  c  =  J 

according  as  a  and  b  are  even  or  odd.  If  b  is  the  larger  number,  ^b  or  i(b  +  i)  is 
the  number  of  partials  which  any  compound  must  have  in  order  to  produce  beats 
when  the  two  tones  composing  the  interval  are  sounded.  If  the  combinational 
tones  are  neglected,  about  double  the  number,  that  is  b,  are  required. 

When  simple  tones  are  sounded  together,  the  beats  arise  from  the  combina- 
tional tones  of  higher  orders.  The  general  expression  for  a  differential  tone  of  a 
higher  order  arising  from  two  tones  with  the  vibrational  numbers  n  and  m  is 
± (an —.few),  and  this  tone  is  then  of  the  (a  +  6—  i)th  order.  Let  the  pitch 
number  of  a  combinational  tone  of  the  (c  +  ^  ~  i)  order  arising  from  the  tones  an 
and  {bn  +  i)  be 

±[{bd-ca)  .  n-^dh], 

and  of  another  of  the  (/+  ^—  i)th  order  be  1 

±[{9b-fa),n  +  g^l 

tlien  both  produce  (gz^d)  ,  ^  beats,  when 

bd  —  ac=^±.{bg  —af) 

a      Q'ln.d. 
or  -==^:?^. 

b       f^LC 

The  lowest  number  of  beats  is  therefore  again  a^,  and  the  lowest  values  of 
c,d,f,  g,  are  found  as  in  the  former  case,  so  that  the  ordinal  numbers  of  combina- 
tioncJ  tones  need  not  exceed  ^(a  +6  —  2),  if  a  and  b  are  both  odd,  or  ^(a  +6—1), 
if  only  one  of  them  is  odd,  the  other  being  even. 

To  what  has  been  said  in  Chap.  VII.,  pp.  154-159, 1  wiU  add  the  following  f 
remarks  on  the  origin  of  combinational  tones. 

Combinational  tones  must  always  arise  when  the  displacement  of  the  vibrating 
particles  from  their  position  of  rest  is  so  large  that  the  force  of  restitution  is  no 
longer  simply  proportional  to  the  displacement.  The  mathematical  theory  of  this 
case  for  a  heavy  vibrating  point  is  given  m  App.  XII. ,  pp.  4 1  ie2  to  41 5^.  The  same 
holds  for  aerial  vibrations  of  finite  magnitude.  The  principles  of  the  theory  are 
given  in  my  essay  on  the  *  Theory  of  Aerial  Vibrations  in  Tuoes  with  Open  Ends,' 
in  Crelle's  Jowmal  fur  Mathematik,  vol.  Ivii.  p.  14.  I  will  here  draw  attention 
to  a  third  case,  where  combinational  tones  may  also  arise  from  infinitely  small 
vibrations.  This  has  already  been  mentioned  in  pp.  is^d-isjd.  It  occurs  with 
sirens  and  harmoniums.  We  have  here  two  openings,  periodically  altering  in 
size,  and  with  a  greater  pressure  of  air  on  one  side  than  on  the  other.  Since  we 
are  dealing  only  with  ver^  small  differences  of  pressure,  we  may  assume,  that  the 
mass  of  the  escaping  air  is  jointly  proportional  to  the  size  of  the  opening  ta,  and  to 
the  difference  of  pressure  p,  so  that 

Digitized  Syi^OOgle 


420  COMBINATIONAL  TONES  OF  SIREN,   ETC.  app.  xvi. 

where  c  is  some  constant.  If  we  now  assume  for  uf  the  simplest  periodic  fdnction 
which  expresses  an  alternate  shutting  and  opening,  namely 

w  =  i4  .  (i  —  sin  2irnt)f 

and  consider  j?  to  be  constant,  that  is,  suppose  uf  to  be  so  small  and  tiie  influx  of 
air  so  copious,  that  the  periodical  loss  through  the  opening  does  not  essentiallj 
alter  the  pressure,  q  will  be  of  the  form 

q  =  B  .  (i  —  sm2irnt) 
where  B=^cAp. 

In  this  case  the  velocity  of  the  motion  of  sound  at  any  place  of  the  space  filled 
with  air,  must  have  a  similar  form,  so  that  only  a  tone  with  the  vibrational  number 
n  can  arise.  But  if  there  is  a  second  greater  opening  of  variable  size,  through 
H  which  there  is  sufficient  escape  of  air  to  render  the  pressure  p  periodically  variable, 
instead  of  being  constant,  as  the  air  passes  out  through  the  other  opening,  that  is, 
if  |7  is  of  the  form 

p=iP  .  (i  —  sin  2irmt), 

then  q  will  have  the  form 

q=icAP  .  (i  —  sin  2irnt)  .  (i  —sin  2wmt) 
zsicAP  .  [i  —  sin  27rwi  — sin  2Trmt  -  ^  cos  2  ir(w  +  n)^+i  ^^^  2ir(m  — n)fj. 

Hence,  in  addition  to  the  two  primary  tones  n  and  m,  there  will  be  also  the 
tones  m-hn  and  m  ^  n,  that  is,  the  two  combinational  tones  of  the  first  order. 

In  reality  the  equations  will  always  be  much  more  complicated  than  those  here 
selected  for  shewing  the  process  in  its  simplest  form.  The  tone  n  will  influence 
the  pressure  p,  as  well  as  the  tone  m ;  even  the  combinational  tones  will  alter  p  ; 
^  and  finally  the  magnitude  of  the  opening  may  not  be  expressible  by  such  a  simple 
periodic  Action  as  we  have  selected  for  ta.  This  will  occasion  not  merely  the 
tones  m,  n,  and  m  +  n,  m  —  n,  to  be  produced,  but  also  their  upper  partiais,  and 
the  combinational  tones  of  those  upper  partiais,  as  may  also  be  observed  in  experi- 
ments. The  complete  theory  of  such  a  case  becomes  extraordinarily  complicated, 
and  hence  the  above  account  of  a  very  simple  case  may  suffice  to  shew  the  nature 
of  the  process. 

I  will  mention  another  experiment  which  may  be  similarly  explained.  The 
lower  box  of  my  double  siren  vibrates  strongly  in  sympathy  with  the  fork  a'  when 
it  is  held  before  the  lower  opening,  and  the  holes  are  idl  covered,  but  not  when  the 
holes  are  open.  On  putting  the  disc  of  the  siren  in  rotation  so  that  the  holes  bib 
alternately  open  and  covered,  the  resonance  of  the  tuning-fork  varies  periodically. 
If  n  is  the  vibrational  number  of  the  fork,  and  m  the  number  of  times  that  a  single 
hole  in  the  box  is  opened,  the  strength  of  the  resonance  will  be  a  periodic  function 
of  the  time,  and  consequently  in  its  simplest  case  equal  to  i  —  sin  2irmt, 
^       Hence  the  vibrational  motion  of  the  air  will  be  of  the  form 

(i  —  sin  2Trmt)  .  sin  2?r?t^  =  sin  27rnt  +  ^  cos  2ir(m  +  n)i  — ^  cos  27r(m— n)^, 

and  consequently  we  hear  the  tones  m  -I-  n,  and  w  —  n  orn  —  m.  If  the  siren  is 
rotated  slowly,  m  will  be  Yery  small,  and  these  tones  being  all  nearly  the  same, 
will  beat.    On  rotating  the  disc  rapidly,  the  ear  separates  them.* 

*  [For  the  whole  subject  of  beats  and  com>      recent  discussions  in  Appendix  XX.  sect.  I«.— 
binational  tones  the  reader  is  referred  to  the      Translator.'] 


Digitized  by 


Google 


APP.  xvu.   JUSTLY  INTONED  INSTRUMENTS  WITH  ONE  MANUAL.    421 


APPENDIX  XVn. 

FLAN  FOB  JUSTLY  INTONED  INBTBUMSNTS  WITH  A  SINGLE   MANUAL. 

(See  p.  319c,  and  note  f.) 

To  arrange  an  organ  or  harmonium  with  twenty-four  tones  to  the  octave  in  such 
a  way  as  to  phiy  in  just  intonation  in  all  keys,  the  tones  of  the  instrument  must 
be  separated  into  four  pairs  of  groups,  thus 


I  a)    / 

a,      ($ 

2  &i    0 

e,       0,? 

3«)    ? 
4  a)    d 

6,      «,) 

lb)    /. 

a       cS 

3  bi    C| 

4  b)    d, 

k  t 

Each  of  these  groups  must  have  a  separate  portvent  from  the  bellows,  and 
valves  must  be  introduced  in  such  a  wav  that  the  wind  may  be  driven  at  pleasure 
either  to  the  right  or  left  group  of  any  horizontal  series.  This  would  not  be  diffi- 
cult on  the  organ.  On  the  harmonium  the  digitals  would  have  to  be  placed  in  a 
different  order  from  the  tongues,  and  consequently  it  would,  as  on  iJie  organ,  be 
necessary  to  have  a  more  complicated  arrangement  for  conducting  the  effect  of 
pressing  down  a  digital  to  the  valva 

Hence  four  valves  are  to  be  arranged  by  stops  or  pedals  in  a  different  way  for 
every  key.  The  following  is  a  table  of  the  arrangement  of  the  stops  for  the  four 
horizontal  series  of  the  tones  named : — 


Major  keys 

Series 

Minor  keys 

X 

3 

3 

4 

Cb* 

b 

a 

a 

(^,b) 
(B,b 

Gfb* 

b 

b 

Db* 

b 

b 

(Cx) 

A^* 

b 

b 

E\>* 

a 

b 

Bb* 

a 

a 

F 

a 

a 

\ 

C 

a 

a 

e\ 

G 

b 

a 

B*    orCb 

D 

b 

b 

2^,5  ♦  or  Gb 

A 

b 

b 

C,«  ♦  or  Db 

E 

b 

b 

G^n*oTAb 

B 

a 

b 

D,«  ♦  or  Eb 

a 

a 

A^Z  *  or  Bb 

ir 

The  minor  keys  which  have  their  names  in  parentheses,  namely  Ei\}j  Bi\},  i^,, 
Ci,  <7i,  Di,  have  a  true  minor  Seventh,  but  too  high  a  leading  tona  [Their 
dominant  chord  has  an  impossible  Pvthagorean  major  Third.]  For  the  six  keys 
marked  with  (*),  the  arrangement  of  the  stops  is  the  same  both  for  major  and 
minor.f 

In  order  to  have  a  complete  series  of  tonics,  each  with  a  perfect  major  and 
minor  form,  it  would  be  necessary  to  cut  off  ai|>,  £,{>,  6i|>,  /i,  Ci,  £1,  from  the  other 
notes,  and  to  allow  them  to  be  replaced  when  needed  by  ^,  e2$,  0$,  <{(,  bjj^,  and 
fJKJl^,  by  means  of  a  fifth  stop.  We  should  thus  have  30  tones  to  the  octave.  By 
drawing  out  this  stop  we  should  have  the  following  system  of  keys : — 

t  [The  series  in  the  first  six  lines  is  the  same  as  in  the  six  last— Tmfw2a^.] 

Digitized  by  VjOOQIC 


42  2 


JUST  INTONATION  IN  SINGING. 


APP.  XVII.  xvin. 


H 


Series  nwrked  with  accented  letters  to  shew 

1 

that  they  are  affected  by  the  fifth  stop 

HAjorkeys 

Minor  keys                  | 

I 

3 

3 

4 

*                    1 

F 

a' 

a' 

a' 

b' 

F 

C 

a' 

a' 

a' 

a' 

C 

O 

b' 

a' 

a' 

a' 

G 

D 

b' 

b' 

a' 

a' 

D 

A 

b' 

b' 

b' 

a' 

A 

E 

b' 

b' 

b' 

b' 

E* 

B 

a' 

b' 

b' 

b' 

A« 

Fl 

a' 

a' 

b' 

b' 

A,t 

CI 

a' 

a' 

a' 

b' 

E,l 

0% 

a' 

a' 

a' 

a' 

B,« 

D% 

b' 

a' 

a' 

a' 

F,t% 

A% 

b' 

b' 

a' 

a' 

Cytn 

E% 

b' 

b' 

b' 

a' 

0,9  8 

To  have  a  complete  series  of  minor  keys,  28  instead  of  30  tones  to  the  octave 
would  be  enough.  They  would  suffice  for  the  12  minor  keys  oiAx,Ei,Bi,  i^ijf  or 
Gt>,  CiJ  or  Dt>,  (?ij  or  -4|>,  Z>iJf  or  E[}j  B,  F,  G,  O  and  D,  and  for  17  major  keys 
from  C|>  mjgor  to  Gj  major,  t 


APPENDIX  XVIIL 


JUST  INTONATION  IN  SINGINQ. 

(See  p.  3266.) 

IT  Since  the  publication  of  the  first  edition  of  this  book,  I  have  had  an  opportunity 
of  seeing  the  Enharmonic  Organ,  constructed  by  General  Perronet  Thompson, t 
which  sdlows  of  performance  in  21  major  and  minor  scales  with  different  tonics 
harmonically  connected.  This  instrument  is  much  more  complicated  than  my 
harmonium.  It  contains  40  pipes  to  the  octave,  and  has  three  distinct  manuals, 
with,  on  the  whole,  65  digitals  to  the  octave,  as  the  same  note  has  to  be  sometimes 
struck  on  two  or  all  of  the  manuals.  This  instrument  allows  of  the  performance 
of  much  more  extensive  modulations  than  my  harmonium,  without  requiring  any 
enharmonic  interchange&  It  is  even  possible  to  execute  tolerably  quick  passages 
and  ornamentations  upon  it,  notwithstanding  its  apparently  involved  fiingenng. 

also  explained.-  Translator.] 

X  [*  On  the  Principles  and  Practice  of  Just 
IntomUion,  with  a  view  to  the  Abolition  of 
Temperament,  and  embodying  the  results  of 
the  Tonic  Sol-fa  Associations,  as  illustrated 
on  ihe  Enharmonic  Organ  .  .  .  presenting  the 
power  of  performing  correctly  in  21  keys  (with 
the  minors  to  the  extent  of  involving  not 
more  than  5  flats),  and  a  correction  for  changes 
of  temperature.  .  .  .  Calculated  for  taking  the 
place  of  the  choir  orgaxk  in  a  cathedral,  and 
learned  by  the  blind  in  six  lessons.  With  an 
Appendix  tracing  the  identity  of  design  with 
the  Enharmonic  of  the  Ancients.*  By  T. 
Perronet  Thompson,  FJK.S.  Ninth  edition, 
1866.  The  exact  compass  of  this  organ  wiU 
be  explained  in  App.  XX.  sect.  F.  No.  6. 
General  Thompson  was  bom  at  Hull,  in  17S3, 
and  died  at  Blackheath,  6  September  1869. 
He  had  been  four  years  in  the  navy  before 
joining  the  army,  and  was  prominent  during 
the  Com  Law  Abolition  agitation.  He  was 
many  years  editor  of  the  Westminsier  Review^ 
and  was  first  returned  to  Parliament  for  HuU 
in  iS^s,— Translator,] 


*  [The  E  minor  has  the  leading  note,  but 
not  the  minor  Seventh.  The  otiier  minor 
keys  have  hoih.^Translator.] 

t  [As  Prof.  Helmholtz  has  retained  this 
Appendix  in  his  4th  German  edition  it  is 
given  in  the  translation.  But  the  scheme 
c|  explained  has  never  been  tried.  The  plan 
for  24  notes  is  impracticable  because  of  the  de- 
fective state  of  the  minor  keys,  and  imperfect 
modulating  power.  It  could  only  be  used 
as  an  experimental  instrument,  and  for  that 
the  double  keyboard  as  explained  on  p.  3166 
suffices.  The  vcJve  arrangement  for  30  notes 
would  be  complicated,  and  even  if  it  could  be 
used  would  still  have  a  very  imperfect  modu- 
lating power.  The  53  division  of  the  octave 
introduced  by  Mr.  Bosanquet,  and  subse- 
quently by  Mr.  Paul  White,  with  fingerboards 
which  have  been  actually  used,  as  explained 
in  App.  XIX.,  and  also  App.  XX.  sect.  F. 
Nos.  8  and  9,  are  so  much  superior  in  mani- 
pulation, musical  effect,  and  power  of  modula- 
tion, that  it  is  unnecessary  to  seek  further. 
In  App.  XX.  sect.  F.  the  other  principal 
methods  that  have  been  actually  tried,  are 


Digitized  by  VjOOQIC 


Aj»p.  xvni. 


JUST  INTONATION  IN  SINGING. 


425 


The  organ  was  erected  in  the  Sunday  School  Chapel,  10  Jewin  Street,  Aldersgate, 
London,*  and  was  built  by  Messrs.  Bobson,  loi  St.  Martin's  Lane,  London.  It 
contains  only  one  stop  of  the  usual  principal  work,  has  Venetian  shutters  form- 
ing a  swell  throughout,  and  is  provided  with  a  peculiar  mechanism  for  correcting 
the  influence  of  temperature  on  the  intonation. 

Mr.  H.  W.  Poole  has  lately  transformed  his  organf  so  as  to  get  rid  of  stops  for 
changing  the  intonation,  and  has  constructed  a  peculiar  arrangement  of  the  digitals, 
which  enables  him  to  play  in  all  keys  with  the  same  fingering.  His  scale  contains 
not  merely  the  just  Fifths  and  Thirds  in  the  series  of  major  chords,  but  also  the 
natural  or  subminor  Sevenths  for  the  tones  of  both  series.  There  are  78  pipes  to 
Die  octave,  and  Fh  has  been  identified  with  J^i,  &c.,  as  upon  my  harmonium.^ 

Successions  of  chords  on  General  Thompson's  instrument  are  extraordinarily 
harmonious,  and,  perhaps,  on  account  of  their  softer  quality  of  tone,  even  more 
surprising  in  their  agreeable  character  than  on  my  harmonium.S  I  had  an  oppor- 
tunity, at  the  same  time,  of  hearing  a  female  singer,  who  had  often  sung  to  it,  per- 
form a  piece  to  the  accompaniment  of  the  enharmonic  organ,  and  her  singing  gave  ^ 
me  a  peculiarly  satisfactory  feehng  of  perfect  certainty  in  intonation,  which  is 
usually  absent  when  a  pianoforte  accompanies.  There  was  also  a  vioUnist**  present 
^vho  had  not  been  much  accustomed  to  play  with  the  organ,  and  accompanied  well- 
Ivnown  airs  by  ear.  He  hit  off  the  intonation  exactly  as  long  as  the  key  remained 
unchanged,  and  it  was  only  in  some  rapid  modulations  that  he  was  not  able  to 
follow  it  perfectly. 

In  London  I  had  also  an  opportunity  of  comparing  the  intonation  of  this 
instrument  with  the  natural  intonation  of  singers  who  had  learned  to  sing  without 
any  instrumental  accompaniment  at  all,  and  are  accustomed  to  follow  their  ear 
alone.  This  was  the  Society  of  Tonic  Sol-£aists,  who  are  spread  in  great  numbers 
(there  were  150,000  in  i862tt)  over  the  large  cities  of  England,  and  whose  great 


*  [Shortly  before  his  death  General  Per- 
Tonet  Thompson  presented  this  organ  to  Mr. 
.Tohn  Garwen,  mentioned  in  note  ff,  below. 
The  General's  executors  had  it  reconstructed 
in  a  schoolroom  at  Plaistow,  Essex.  It  was 
afterwards  exhibited  at  the  Scientific  Loan 
Exhibition  at  South  Kensington  Museum  in 
1876,  and  has  remained  there  ever  since,  at 
the  top  of  the  staircase  leading  to  room  Q  of 
the  Science  Collections. — Translator,] 

t  Silliman's  American  Journal  of  Science 
and  Arts,  vol.  xliv.,  July  1867.  [In  its  origi- 
nal form  the  instrument,  with  an  ordinary 
keyboard  and  pedals,  was  termed  the  Ett- 
hannonic  Organ,  and  is  described  in  Silliman's 
Journal,  vol.  ix.  p.  209,  for  May  1850.  The 
new  fingerboard  is  figured  and  described  here- 
after, App.  XX.  sect.  F.  No.  7  .—Translator.] 

X  [The  text  is  in  error.  There  are  100  not 
78  pipes  to  the  octave,  and  Ei  is  not  identified 
with  Fb,— Translator.] 

§  [*  On  organs  of  many  stops,  one  or  more 
ought  certainly  to  be  tuned  with  mathemati- 
cally correct  intonation,  on  account  of  their 
wonderful  effect,  to  be  employed  (of  course 
without  using  any  others  at  the  same  time) 
as  the  music  of  the  spheres  {als  Oesang  der 
Sphdren).  It  is  impossible  to  form  any  notion 
of  the  effect  of  a  chord  in  mathematically  just 
intonation,  without  having  heard  it.  I  have 
such  a  one  to  compare  with  the  others.  Every 
one  who  hears  it  expresses  his  delight  and 
surprise  at  a  correctness  of  intonation  that  it 
does  one  good  to  hear  {Jeder,  der  thn  hOrt, 
gpricht  sei/n  frohes  Erstaunen  Uber  diese 
toohllhuende  Reinheit  aus).'— Scheibler,  Ueber 
maihematische  Stimmung,  Temperaturen  und 
Orgelstimmung  nach  Vibrations-Differenzen 
Oder  Stdssen,  1838.  I  have  given  the  original 
words  of  the  last  German  sentence,  as  it  was 
impossible  to  do  justice  to  its  homely  force  in 


any  translation.  Every  one  who  has  heard 
just  intonation  will  understand  it.— Trans- 
lator.] 

*♦  [A  blind  man,  who  had  therefore  no  ir 
notes  to  guide  him.  I  had  the  pleasure  of 
taking  Professor  Helmholtz  to  hear  the  organ 
on  this  occasion  (20  April  1864),  and  can  corro- 
borate his  statements.  Unfortunately  the 
proper  blind  organist  was  not  present.  It  is 
to  this  lady  that  General  Thompson  dedicates 
his  little  book,  already  recited,  in  these  words : 
*To  Miss  E.  S.  Northcote,  Organist  of  St. 
Anne  and  St.  Agnes,  St.  Martin 's-le-Grand. 
In  conunemoration  of  the  talent  by  which, 
after  six  lessons,  she  was  able  to  perform  in 
public  on  the  enharmonic  organ  with  40  sounds 
to  the  octave ;  thereby  settling  the  question  of 
the  practicability  of  just  intonation  on  keyed 
instruments,  and  realising  the  visions  of  Guido 
and  Mersenne,  and  the  harmonists  of  classical 
antiquity.'—  Translator.] 

ff  [The  20  years  which  have  elapsed  since 
Prof.  Helmholtz 's  first  acquaintance  with  the  ^ 
Tonic  Sol-fa  movement  have  made  a  struggling 
system,  slowly  elaborated  by  a  Congregation- 
fUist  minister  in  connection  with  his  ministry, 
into  a  great  national  system  of  teaching  sing- 
ing. And  as  the  system  had  the  cordial 
approval  of  Prof.  Helmholtz  (see  note  p.  427^), 
I  feel  justified  in  adding  a  short  account  of 
its  origin,  progress,  and  present  condition. 
In  18 1 2  the  two  Miss  Glovers,  daughters  of 
a  clergyman  of  Norwich^  then,  young  women, 
now  both  dead  at  a  very  advanced  age,  in- 
vented and  introduced  into  the  schools  under 
their  superintendence  a  new  sol-fa  system, 
based  upon  the  'movable  doh,*  that  is,  the 
use  of  doh  as  the  name  of  the  keynote,  what- 
ever that  might  be.  This  was  little  known 
beyond  the  town  where  it  was  used,  but 
was  published  about  1827  as  a  Scheme  for 


Digitized  by  VjOOQlC 


434 


JUST  INTONATION  IN  SINGING. 


▲PP.  xynL 


progress  is  of  much  importance  for  the  theory  of  music.    The  Tonic  Sol-fusts  re- 
present the  tones  of  the  major  scale  by  the  syllables  Do,  Be,  Mi,  Fa,  So,  La^  Ti, 


rendering  Psalmody  Congregational,  and  passed 
through  three  editions.  About  1841  John 
Curwen,  then  an  unmarried  GongregationcJist 
minister  (bom  14  November  1816  at  Heck- 
mondwike,  Yorks),  visited  the  school,  and  at 
once  saw  that  Miss  Glover's  scheme  gave  him 
the  instrument  he  desired  for  his  own  work. 
In  1845  ^G  married,  and  he  and  his  wife 
struggled — it  was  a  real  and  severe  struggle, 
against  small  means — to  make  this  system 
l^own  and  active.  In  the  course  of  working 
it  out  various  improvements  suggested  them- 
selves, and  the  Tonic  Sol-fa  system,  as  he 
termed  it,  is  not  precisely  the  same  as  Miss 

^  Glover*s;  it  is  essentially  John  Ourwen's. 
Thus  Miss  Glover*8  scheme  (as  she  says  in  a 
MS.  preface  in  1862  to  the  2nd  edition  of  the 
description  of  her  Harmonicon,  in  the  Science 
Collections,  South  Kensington  Museum)  was 
founded  on  temperament ;  Gurwen*s  on  just 
intonation ;  and  the  alterations  that  this 
change  involved  were  many  and  laborious. 
Here  Curwen  was,  I  believe,  much  assisted  by 
the  personal  friendship  of  General  Perronet 
Thompson,  whose  works  he  constantly  quoted 
in  the  first  book  he  issued,  Singing  for  Schools 
and  Congregations,  1843-8.  A  remarkable 
power  of  methodising,  systeraatising,  and 
teaching,  of  making  fnencb  and  co-workers, 
and  of  utilising  suggestions  carried  everything 
before  it — at  last.  But  the  work  was  long, 
and  the  opposition  strong.  There  was  only  an 
*  Association '  when  Prof.  Helmholtz  made  the 

^[  acquaintance  of  the  Tonic  Sol-fa  system.  But 
the  Association  grew  to  be  a  *  College,*  which 
held  its  first  *  summer  term '  on  10  July  1876, 
having  been  '  incorporated  '  on  26  June  1875, 
and  there  were  in  1884,  1420  *  Shareholders  ' 
in  this  College,  which  opened  its  *  Buildings ' 
(at  Forest  Gate,  London,  E.)  on  5  July  1879. 
John  Curwen  lived  long  enough  to  see  the 
opening  and  to  preside  at  the  unveiling  of  Miss 
Glover's  portrait  in  it,  never  having  neglected 
to  own  his  obligations  to  her  initiative.  On 
a  stone  at  the  entrance  of  the  present  College 
building  he  placed  this  inscription :  *  This 
stone  was  laid  by  John  Curwen,  May  14,  1879, 
in  memory  of  Miss  Sarah  Glover,  on  whose 
**  Scheme  for  rendering  Psalmody  Congrega- 
tional "  the  Tonic  Sol-fa  method  was  founded.' 
John  Curwen  died  26  May  1880,  of  weakness 

^  of  the  heart.  His  eldest  son,  John  Spencer 
Curwen,  Associate  of  the  Boyal  Academy  of 
Music,  has  been  since  that  date  annually 
elected  as  President  of  the  College.  The  work 
of  the  College  is  chiefly  examinational,  carrying 
on  classes  by  post  in  the  various  branches  of 
music,  and  granting  certificates  shewing  various 
degrees  of  attainment,  on  the  authority  of  duly 
appointed  examiners.  From  1858  to  1884  the 
numbers  of  these  certificates  granted  have 
been:  Junior,  52,000;  Elementary,  167,000; 
Intermediate,  44,000;  Matriculation,  3350; 
Advanced,  520 ;  Musical  Theory  (including 
Harmony,  Composition,  Form,  Expression, 
Acoustics,  (fee),  8200;  total  275,070,  as  the 
Secretary  informs  me.  During  the  summer 
there  is  always  a  term  for  the  special  vvod  voce 
instruction  of  teachers.  Of  course  large  classes 
are  constantly  going  on  everywhere.  I  qoote 
the  following  from  a  letter  dftted  15  October 


1884,  written  by  John  Spencer  Curwen  to  the 
Editor  of  the  Times  :— 

At  the  most  modest  estimate,  daring  the  y> 
years  our  system  has  been  at  work,  we  have  taognt 
at  least  the  elements  of  mnsic  to  fbur  million  per- 
sons. There  are  now,  in  the  elementaiy  aehooh  of 
the  United  Kingdom,  about  one  mUlion  children 
learning  to  sing  at  sight  npon  our  system.  The 
Tonic  Sol-fa  College  has  28  difierent  kinds  and 
grades  of  mnsical  ezaminationa,  and  these  were 
passed  last  year  b^  18,716  persons.  Eveiy  ex- 
amination includes  individmd  tests  in  «tigiwp  at 
sight.  We  have  between  4000  and  5000  tcAoen 
at  work,  and  at  the  present  time  they  have  under 
instruction  some  200,000  adults^  in  addition  to  the 
children  already  mentioned.  I  lately  inquired  of 
16  of  our  most  active  professional  teacMrs  how 
many  pupils,  adults  and  children,  they  were  in- 
structing per  week  in  their  classes.  The  nomber 
proved  to  be  61,051.  We  have  a  weJl-ornudaed 
movement.  During  the  last  four  yean  I  hare 
attended  166  meetings  in  the  length  and  breadth 
of  the  kingdom,  my  travels  extending  over  13,000 
miles,  and  ranging  from  Plymouth  to  LondoniJerry, 
firom  Inverness  to  Norwich.  These  meetings,  at 
which  demonstrations  of  mnsical  education  are 
invariably  given,  have  been  attended  b^  at  least 
zoo/xx>  people.  I  have  further  travelled  m  France. 
Germany,  Austria,  and  Switzerland,  stud3rin|^  the 
condition  of  popular  musical  instruction  in  s<£ooU» 
singing  clubs,  &C.,  so  that  we  may  bring  our  prac- 
tice up  to  the  best  continental  mo'deU.  The  quan- 
tity of  music  printed  in  the  Tonic  Sol-fa  notation 
is  enormous,  and  is  increasing  very  rapidly.  Two- 
thirds  of  our  pupils,  having  been  groundiKl  in  our 
noution,  go  on  to  learn  the  ordinaiy  staff  nota- 
tion, and  prove  themselves  excellent  readers  ef 
that  notation. 

With  regard  to  teaching  musio  in  8oh<x)iU, 
the  following  is  compiled  from  the  papen 
issued  by  the  Educational  Department  in 
1884,  England  and  Wales  C.  3941,  and  Soot- 
land  C.  394^.  They  refer  to  27,330  onbordi- 
nate  educational  departments  for  England  and 
Wales.  Of  these,  21,743  teach  mnsio  by  ear 
only ;  1429  by  the  staff  or  ordinary  notation  ; 
3871  by  Tonic  Sol-fa;  32  by  both  systems; 
and  2161  in  some  other  way.  For  SootlAnd 
there  are  3403  subordinate  departments,  of 
which  1238  teach  by  ear  only ;  8  by  the 
late  Dr.  Hullah's  modification  of  Wilhelm*8 
method,  1746  by  Tonic  Sol-fa;  117  by  old 
notation  with  movable  doh  (for  which  many 
teasers  have  a  strong  predilection),  and  7  by 
more  than  one  system.  There  are  94  depart- 
ments in  England,  and  277  in  Scotland  miAii^g 
no  returns.  These  returns  shew  that  Toni« 
Sol-fa  is  the  national  system  of  teaching 
musio  by  note  in  the  primary  sohooLs  of  Eng- 
land and  Scotland  at  the  present  day. 

John  Curwen  having  started  his  system 
from  purely  philanthropic  motives,  gladly 
placed  his  notation  at  the  disposal  of  all  who 
liked  to  use  it.  A  strong  proof  of  the  soooesa 
of  his  system  is  furnished  by  the  fact  that  aJl 
the  principal  London  publi^ers  have  availed 
themselves  of  this  permission.  Ctonnod's  Re- 
demption and  Mackenzie's  Rose  of  Sharon  are 
among  the  latest  additions  to  the  Tonio 
Sol-fa  repertoire.  It  is  estimated  that  at  the 
present  time  there  are  40,000  pages  of  mnsifl 
printed  and  published  in  this  latter  notation. 
But  the  ednoatiottid  works  on  musio  and  the 
system  are  the  private  property  of  the  firm  of 


Digitized  by  VjOOQlC 


APP.  xvni. 


JUST  INTONATION  IN   SINGING. 


425 


Do.  where  Do  is  always  the  tonic  [vowels  as  in  Italian].  Their  vocal  music  is  not 
written  in  ordinary  musical  notation,  but  is  printed  with  common  types,  the  initial 
letters  of  the  above  words  representing  the  pitch.* 

When  the  tonic  is  changed  in  modulations,  the  notation  is  also  changed.  The 
n&w  tonic  is  now  called  Do,  and  the  change  is  pointed  out  in  the  notation  by  giving 
two  different  marks  to  the  note  on  which  it  occurs,  one  belonging  to  the  old,  and 
one  to  the  new  key.  This  notation,  therefore,  gives  the  very  first  place  to  repre- 
senting the  relation  of  every  note  to  the  tonic,  while  the  absolute  pitch  in  which 
the  piece  has  to  be  performed  is  marked  at  the  commencement  only.  Since  the 
intervals  of  the  natural  major  scale  are  transferred  to  each  new  tonic  as  it  arises 
in  the  course  of  modulation,  all  keys  are  performed  without  tempering  the  inter- 
nals. That  in  the  modulation  from  C  major  to  Q  major,  the  Mi  (or  hx)  of  the 
second  scale  answers  precisely  to  the  Ti  of  the  first  is  not  indicated  in  the  nota- 
tion, and  is  only  taught  in  the  further  course  of  instruction.  Hence  the  pupil  has 
no  inducement  to  confase  a  with  a^.f  H 


John  Cnrwen  &  Sons,  and  are  of  such  a  remark- 
able character  that  a  gold  medal  was  awarded 
for  them  at  the  International  Health  and  Edu- 
cation Exhibition  at  South  Kensington  in  1884. 
It  would  indeed  be  difficult  to  find  so  much 
information  on  music  and  the  method  of 
teaching  it  (in  both  notations),  so  succinctly 
and  plainly  given,  and  at  so  cheap  a  rate,  as 
in  the  late  John  Curwen's  T6a4iher^8  Manual, 
standard  Course,  Miisical  Theory,  How  to 
Observe  Hannony,  How  to  Read  Music,  not  to 
mention  the  very  large  number  of  books  and 
music  intended  for  immediate  class  use.  John 
Curwen's  especial  desire  was  to  teach  *  the 
thing  music,'  as  he  words  it,  and  the  peculiar 
means  which  he  elaborated  for  this  purpose, 
he  valued  only  because  it  proved  effectual  for 
that  purpose. 

As  one  who  was  personally  acquainted 
with  John  Curwen  and  his  work  for  a  quarter 
of  a  century,  I  may  be  permitted  to  give  this 
testimony,  and  to  refer  all  those  who  would 
learn  the  history  of  this  successful  musical 
educationaUst  to  the  Memorials  of  John  Cur- 
wen, compiled  by  his  son,  J.  Spencer  Curwen, 
iSHz,— Translator.] 

*  [Great  care  has  also  been  bestowed  on 
the  representation  of  rhythm,  and  exercises  in 
rhythm  form  an  important  part  of  the  Stand- 
ard Course  and  the  practice  of  Tonic  Sol-fa 
teachers.  —Translator.] 

t  [In  a  footnote  to  this  passage  Prof.  Helm- 
holtz  gives  a  list  of  the  Tonic  Sol-fa  works, 
which  is  superseded  by  the  note  I  have  in- 
serted above,  and  at  the  end  of  it  he  says:] 
In  France  singing  is  taught  by  the  Oalin- 
Paris-Chcvi  system,  on  similar  principles  and 
with  the  help  of  a  similar  notation.  [This 
statement  is  misleading.  Neither  principles 
nor  notation  are  alike.  In  1818  P.  Galin, 
*  Instituteur  k  TEcole  des  Sourds-Muets  de 
Bordeaux,'  published  his  Exposition  d*une 
Nouvelle  MUhode  pour  VEnseignement  de  la 
Musique,  It  follows  from  p.  162  of  his  book 
(3rd  ed.  1863,  reprinted  by  ]6mile  Chev6)  that 
Galin  adopted  as  his  normal  intonation  Huy- 
ghens's  cycle  of  31  divisions  of  the  octave, 
which  closely  represents  the  meantone  tem- 
perament (see  App.  XX.  sect.  A.  art.  22,  ii.), 
although  Galin  did  not  seem  to  be  acquainted 
with  it  under  that  name,  and  seems  to  an- 
nounce as  his  own  discovery  {ibid.  p.  80,  and 
especially  p.  107)  what  was  in  fact  Huyghens's 
more  than  120  years  previously  :  viz.  that  I 
of  a  whole  Tone  » j^  major  Semitone  «« J  of  a 


minor  Semitone,  but  the  ourions  thing  is  that 
he  considers  the  resulting  flat  Fifth  of  696773 
cents  to  be  correct,  and  &e  Fifth  with  701*955 
cents  from  the  ratio  of  2  :  3  to  be  wrong. 
This  is  enough  to  shew  how  widely  Galin's 
principles  differed  from  Curwen's.  The  nota- 
tion of  intervals  which  Galin  used  was  Bous- 
seau's  numerical  expression  of  the  major  scale 
as  I  2  3  4  5  6  7,  indicating  a  rising  Octave  by 
overdotting  and  a  falling  by  underdotting, 
but  calling  the  figures  ut  ri  mi  fa  sol  la  si. 
Here  the  only  resemblance  is  the  movable  ut 
{  =  doh),  as  distinguished  from  the  usual 
French  custom  of  making  ut^C.  In  mark- 
ing sharps  and  flats  and  time  the  difference  is 
greater,  but  need  not  be  pursued.  We  should 
observe,  however,  that  on  this  system,  as  Galin 
expressly  states  (p.  Si),gZ  is  flatter  than  ab.  % 
Galin  was  bom  16  Dec.  1786,  and  died  30 
Aug.  1822.  His  pupUs,  and  especially  Aim6 
a^d  Nanine  Paris  (the  latter  of  whom  married 
Emile  Chev6,  a  surgeon),  continued  to  teach 
his  system,  and  supplied  it  with  text-books. 
The  principal  one  is  MUhode  Slementaire  par 
Mine.  Chev^  (Nanine  Paris).  La  partie  thio- 
rique  de  cet  ouvrage  est  ridigie  par  J^mile 
Chevi.  In  this  theoretical  part,  p.  292, 1  find 
that  Chev6  imagined  Galin  to  have  called  his 
single  division  half  of  a  minor  Second,  whereas 
he  says,  as  above,  that  it  was  half  of  a  minor 
Semitone,  which  is  totally  different.  The  con- 
sequence is  that  Chev6  makes  Galin's  scale  a 
division  of  the  octave  into  29  divisions,  in- 
stead of  31 ,  and  hence  he  obtained  a  sharp  Fifth 
of  703*46  cents,  a  very  sharp  major  Third  of 
413*8  cents,  much  sharper  than  the  Pytha- 
gorean (App.  XX.  sect.  A.  art.  23,  vi.).  If  he  <r 
could  have  tuned  an  harmonium  to  this  major 
scale  and  played  the  major  chords,  he  would 
have  been  scared  at  the  result.  He  makes  gt 
much  sharper  than  ab;  his  ab  was  indeed 
flatter  and  gt  sharper  than  on  the  Pytha- 
gorean system.  It  is  evident  that  his  pupils 
when  they  sang  in  chorus  could  not  have 
used  his  theoretical  scale.  Hence  his  prin- 
ciples were  entirely  different  from  Curwen's. 
The  notation  remained  the  same  as  with  Galin, 
sharps  and  flats  being  denoted  by  acute  and 
grave  accents  drawn  through  the  stems  of  the 
figures,  but  their  meaning  was  altogether  dif- 
ferent. Also  he  retained  the  movable  ut  of 
Galin,  and  on  p.  327  he  made  out  a  general 
table  of  the  relation  of  modulations,  which  re- 
sembles my  Duodenarinm,  App.  XX.  sect  E.  art. 
18.    M.  Aim^  Paris  also  introduced  a  plan  for 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


426 


JUST  INTONATION  IN   SINGING. 


APP.    XVlUr 


It  is  impossible  not  to  acknowledge  that  this  method  of  notatioo  has  the  great 
advantage  to  the  singer  of  giving  prominence  to  what  is  of  the  greatest  im- 
portance to  him,  namely,  the  relation  of  each  tone  to  the  tonic.  It  is  odIj  a 
few  persons,  unusually  gifted,  who  are  able  to  fix  in  their  mind,  and  re-discover 
absolute  pitches,  when  other  tones  are  sounded  at  the  same  time.  But  the  ordinary 
notation*  gives  directly  nothing  but  absolute  pitch,  and  that  too  only  for  tempered 
intonation.  Any  one  who  has  frequently  sung  at  sight  is  aware  how  much  earner 
it  is  to  do  so  from  a  pianoforte  vocal  score,  in  which  the  harmony  is  shewn,  than 
from  the  separate  voice  part.  In  the  first  case  it  is  easy  to  see  whether  the  note  to 
be  sung  is  the  root.  Third,  Fifth,  or  dissonance  of  the  chord  which  occurs,  and  it  is 
then  comparatively  easy  to  find  one*s  way ;  f  in  the  second  case  the  only  resource 
of  the  singer  is  to  go  up  and  down  by  intervals  as  well  as  he  can,  and  trust  to 
the  accompanying  instruments  and  the  other  voices  to  force  his  own  to  the  right 
pitch. 

Now  the  instruction  conveyed  to  a  singer  who  is  familiar  with  musical  theory 
IF  by  an  examination  of  the  pianoforte  vocal  score,  is  conveyed  by  the  notation  itself 
of  the  Tonic  Sol-faist  even  to  the  uninstructed.  I  have  convinced  myself  that  by 
using  this  notation,  it  is  much  easier  to  sing  from  the  separate  part  than  in 
ordinary  musical  notation,  and  I  had  an  opportunity  when  in  one  of  the  primary 
schools  in  London,  of  hearing  more  than  forty  children  of  between  eight  and 


marking  modulations  which  has  a  great  re- 
semblance to  John  Curwen's  *  bridge  tone,' 
but  both  plans  were  absolutely  independent. 
The  *  langue  des  durtes '  of  Aim6  Paris  was, 
however,  avowedly  adapted  to  the  Tonic  Sol-fa 
system  by  John  Curwen.  Both  M.  and  Mme. 
K.  Chev6  are  dead,  and  after  some  time  their 
son  Amand  Chev6  revived  the  system,  which 
has  had  great  success  in  France,  and  gained 
many^  prizes  in  choral  competitions,  shewing 
that  Emile  Chevy's  theoretical  scale  could  not 
have  been  adopted.    From  a  correspondence  I 

V  had  with  M.  Amand  Chev6  I  found  he  did 
not  hold  with  his  father's  29  division  of  the 
octave,  but  adopted  the  53  division  (not 
however  as  representing  just  intonation  with 
a  major  Third  of  17  degrees  or  384*9  cents, 
but)  as  representing  the  Pythagorean  intona- 
tion with  a  major  Third  of  18  degrees  or  407*5 
cents  (App.  XX.  sect.  A.  art.  22,  iii.).  As  this 
would  be  frightful  in  part  singing,  it  is  pro- 
bable that  his  pupils,  although  strictly  taught 
to  make  g1^  sharper  than  ab  (indeed  to  make 
the  intervals  ^  to  ab  and  gi  io  a  identical, 
each  containing  4  degrees  or  90*6  cents,  with 
an  interval  of  i  degree  or  22*6  cents  between 
them),  in  choral  singing  insensibly  use^  the 
equal  temperament  which  Galin  and  Emile 
Chev6  for  different  reasons  inveighed  against. 
At  any  rate  the  Galin-Pari8-Chev6  system, 

^  clever  and  successful  as  it  is,  is  after  til  and 
was  from  the  first  a  tempered  system,  and  in 
its  Chev6  form  a  ^theoretically)  very  badly 
tempered  system,  ana  hence  not  in  the  slightest 
degree  simUar  in  principle  to  the  Tonic  Sol-fa, 
which  as  taught  by  John  Curwen  was  always  a 
system  of  just  intonation.  Another  immense 
difference  must  be  noted.  Curwen  founds 
ever}'thing  upon  the  major  chord  do  mi  so  at 
all  pitches,  then  proceeds  to  its  dominant 
80  ti  re,  and  finally  to  its  subdominant 
fa  la  do,  in.  every  case  drawing  attention  to 
the  character  of  the  notes  in  the  scale.  The 
Chev6  system  began  by  teaching  the  melody, 
ut  re  mi  fa  sol,  and  not  advancing  till  this 
melody  was  thoroughly  impressed  on  the  mind 
of  the  pupil  for  any  ut,  taken  backwards  or 
forwards,  or  stopping  at  any  note  and  begin- 
ning again  at  that  note.  Afterwards  the  sys- 
tem took  the  melody  tU}  si  la  sol,  and  treated 


it  in  the  same  way.  Finally  the  two  were 
united  as  ut  re  mi  fa  sol  la  si  iit^.  On  these 
melodies  all  is  founded,  and  the  pupil  is  told 
to  take  any  other  intervals  by  imagining  the 
intermediate  notes,  without  uttering  them^ 
thus  (the  notes  in  roman  letters  being  merely 
imagined),  ut  re  mi  fa  sol  la  si  ut.  This  is 
developed  in  Mme.  Chev6*s  Science  et  Art  de 
Vintonation,  th6orie  et  pratique,  systime  des 
points  d^appui,  1868.  On  the  title-page  she 
says :  *  Les  grands  ressorts  de  notre  methods, 
pour  P^tude  de  Pintonation,  consistent  en 
ceci:  i^  Chercher  les  sons  un  a  un  et  les 
^mettre  aussi  un  d  un,  en  les  d^tachant  les 
uns  des  autres.  Hors  de  Ik  point  de  sncc^ 
possible.  2^  Se  servir  de  deux  rapports  que 
I'on  connait,  pour  trouver  un  troisi^me  rapport 
qu^on  ignore ;  c^est-i-dire,  aller  du  conna  i 
Pinconnu ;  ce  qui  conduit  k  penser  par  degr^ 
conjoints,  en  ohantant  par  degr^s  disjoints,* 
May  1868.  The  two  systems  of  Chev6  and 
Curwen  ar6  therefore  distinct  in  principle, 
value  of  the  signs,  form  of  the  signs,  notation 
of  rhythm,  and  mode  of  teaching.  They  are 
alike  in  being  taught  without  an  instrument, 
but  for  very  different  reasons;  in  the  Tonic 
Sol-fa  to  allow  just  intonation  to  become  the 
pupiPs  guide  ;  in  the  ChevS  to  allow  of  taldng 
gt  sharper  than  ab,  and  to  make  «  to/  the 
same  as  gZ  to  a,  but  different  from  ab  to  a. 
They  are  also  alike  in  having  a  movable  do  or 
ut,  a  very  ancient  device.  And  also  alike  in 
their  nomenclature  of  lengths,  *  langue  des 
dur^es,'  which  was  an  original  invention  of 
Aim6  Paris.  As  to  priority  of  invention.  Miss 
Glover  taught  her  system  in  18 12,  Galin  pub- 
lished his  in  1818.  Both  used  tempered  sys- 
tems.  -Translator  J] 

♦  [Usually  called  *  the  Staff  Notation '  ot 
•  the  Old  Notation  *  by  the  Tonic  Sol-faists  by 
way  of  distinction.— Traniiator.] 

f  [After  a  pupil  has  thoroughly  acquired 
music  on  the  Sol-fa  notation,  it  be«)me8  part 
of  his  duty  to  learn  the  other,  and  a  coarse  of 
instruction  has  been  prepared  for  this  purpose 
by  Mr.  Curwen,  which  when  properly  mastered 
(a  comparatively  easy  task)  puts  the  pupil  in 
a  concUtion  to  sing  at  sight  from  the  old 
notation  as  readily  as  from  the  new. — Trang" 
lator.] 


Digitized  by  V^OOQIC 


APP.  XVIII. 


JUST  INTONATION  IN   SINGING. 


427 


twelve  years  of  age,  that  performed  singing  exercises  in  a  manner  that  astonished 
me  ('mich  in  Erstaunen  setzten*)  by  the  certainty  with  which  they  read  the 
notes,  and  by  the  accuracy  of  their  intonation.  *  Every  year  the  London  schools 
of  Sol-fedsts  are  accustomed  to  give  a  concert  of  two  to  three  thousand  children's 
voices  in  the  Crystal  Palace  at  Sydenham,  which,  I  have  been  assured  by  persons 
who  understand  music,  makes  the  best  impression  on  the  audience  by  the  har- 
moniousness  and  exactness  of  its  execution.f 

The  Tonic  Sol-faists,  then,  sing  by  natural,  and  not  by  tempered  intervals. 
When  their  choirs  are  accompanied  by  a  tempered  organ,  there  are  marked  differ- 
ences and  disturbances,  whereas  they  are  in  perfect  unison  with  General  Thomp- 
son's Enharmonic  Organ.  Many  expressions  used  are  very  characteristic.  A 
young  girl  had  to  sing  a  solo  in  F  minor,  and  took  it  home  to  study  it  at  her  piano- 
forte. When  she  returned  she  said  that  the  A\}  and  IJ\}  on  her  piano  were  all 
wrong.  These  are  the  Third  and  Sixth  of  the  key  in  which  the  deviation  of 
tempered  from  just  intonation  is  most  marked.  Another  girl  was  so  charmed  with 
the  Enharmonic  Organ  that  she  remained  practising  for  three  hours  in  succession,  ^ 
declaring  that  it  was  pleasant  to  be  able  to  play  real  notes.  Generally  in  a  large 
number  of  cases,  young  people  who  have  learned  to  sing  by  the  Sol-fa  method, 
find  out  by  themselves,  without  any  instruction,  how  to  use  the  complicated 
manuals  of  the  Enharmonic  Organ,  and  always  select  the  proper  intervals. 

Singers  find  that  it  is  easier  to  sing  to  the  accompaniment  of  this  organ,  and 
also  that  they  do  not  hear  the  instrument  while  they  are  singing,  because  it  is  in 
perfect  harmony  with  their  voice  and  makes  no  beats. 


*  [On  20  April  1864,  after  we  had  heard 
Gen.  Ferronet  Thompson's  organ,  I  had  the 
pleasure  of  taking  Prof.  Helmholtz  to  hear 
the  singing  of  the  children  in  the  British  and 
Foreign  School  here  alluded  to,  which  was 
situate  behind  the  chapel  in  Tottenham  Court 
Boad.  The  master  of  the  school,  Mr.  Gardi- 
ner, was  a  very  good  Tonic  Sol-fa  teacher,  but 
the  children  were  those  who  ordinarily  at- 
tended (about  forty  were  then  present)  and 
had  received  only  ordinary  instruction.  After 
hearing  them  sing  a  few  tunes  in  parts,  from 
the  Tonic  Sol-fa  notation,  Prof.  Helmholtz 
himself  '  pointed '  out  an  air  on  the  '  modu- 
lator '  or  scale  drawn  out  large  on  a  chart, 
from  which  the  pupils  learn  to  sing  (that  is, 
by  means  of  a  pointer  shewed  the  Tonic  Sol-fa 
names  of  the  tones  the  children  were  to  sing), 
and  the  class  followed  in  unison  at  sight. 
Then,  on  the  suggestion  of  Mr.  Gardiner,  the 
class  was  divided  into  two  sections,  and  Prof. 
Helmholtz  pointed  a  piece  in  two  parts,  one 
with  each  hand,  while  the  class  took  them  at 
sight  Of  course  the  piece  was  simple,  but 
the  dissonance  of  a  Semitone  was  purposely 
introduced  in  one  place  between  the  parts, 
and  Prof.  Helmholtz  was  delighted  at  the 
firmness  and  correctness  with  which  the  chil- 
dren took  it.  I  recollect  his  saying  to  me 
afterwards,  *We  could  not  do  that  in  Ger- 
many 1  '  meaning,  as  he  subsequently  ex- 
plained, that  there  was  no  German  system  of 
teaching  to  sing  which  could  produce  such 
results  on  such  materials.  The  following  is 
an  extract  of  a  letter  from  Prof.  Helmholtz  to 
Mr.  Curwen  printed  on  p.  159  of  the  Memo- 
rials t  dated  21  April  1864,  the  day  after  his 
visit  to  the  class  :  '  We  were  really  surprised 
by  the  readiness  and  surety  [certainty]  with 
which  the  children  succeeded  in  reading  music 
that  they  did  not  know  before,  and  in  follow- 
ing a  series  of  notes  which  were  indicated  to 
them  on  their  modulatory  board  [modulator]. 
I  think  that  what  I  saw  shewed  the  complete 
success  of  your  system,  and  I  was  peculiarly 
interested  by  it,  because  during  my  researches 


in  musical  acoustics  I  came  from  theoretical 
reasons  to  the  conviction  that  this  was  the 
natural  way  of  learning  music,  but  I  did  not 
know  that  it  had  been  carried  out  in  England 
with  such  beautiful  results.' — Translator.'] 

t  [I  am  informed  by  the  Secretary  of  the 
Tonic  Sol-fa  College  that  the  first  Crystal 
Palace  Festival  of  the  Tonic  Sol-faists  was 
held  on  2  September  1857,  with  a  choir  of  ^T 
about  3200  children  and  300  adults.  These 
concerts  have  been  continued  year  by  year  to 
the  present  time.  For  many  years  two  con- 
certs were  given,  one  juvenile  and  one  adult, 
the  singers  varying  in  number  from  3500  to 
5000.  Some  of  these  performances  were  so 
popular  that  a  repetition  was  given  a  few  weeks 
later.  The  plan  of  testing  the  great  choirs  in 
sight  singing  was  first  tried  at  the  Festival  on 
14  August  1867,  at  which  I  was  present,  when 
an  anthem  specially  written  for  the  occasion 
by  Mr.  (now  Sir)  G.  A.  Macfarren  (Professor  of 
Music  at  the  University  of  Cambridge,  and  Prin- 
cipal of  the  Royal  Academy  of  Music)  was  sung 
by  a  choir  of  4500  voices.  Of  the  performance 
of  this  anthem  Mr.  Macfarren  wrote  a  short 
time  after  in  the  Comhill  Magazine  thus : 
*  A  piece  of  music  which  had  been  composed 
for  the  occasion,  and  had  not  until  then  been  flr 
seen  by  human  eyes  save  those  of  the  writer 
and  the  printers,  was  handed  forth  to  the  mem- 
bers of  the  chorus  there  present,  and  then, 
before  an  audience  furnished  at  the  same  time 
with  copies  to  test  the  accuracy  of  the  per- 
formance, forty-five  hundred  singers  sang  it  at 
first  sight  in  a  manner  to  fulfil  the  highest 
requirements  of  the  severest  judges.*  Mr. 
Macfarren  was  himself  present,  and  publicly 
expressed  his  own  satisfaction. 

Sight-singing  tests  have  been  given  almost 
every  year  since,  and  always  with  the  same 
success.  They  have  become  a  common  part  of 
public  concerts  intended  as  *  demonstrations,' 
and  are  regarded  by  Tonic  Sol-faists  as  no 
more  extraordinary  than  reading  the  words 
at  sight  would  be  considered. — Translator.'] 


Digitized  by  VjOOQIC 


428  JUST  INTONATION  IN  SINGING.  app,  xvm. 

* 

I  have  myself  observed,  that  singers  accustomed  to  a  pianoforte  accompani- 
ment, when  they  sang  a  simple  melody  to  my  justly  intoned  harmonium,  san^ 
natural  Thirds  and  Sixths,  not  tempered,  nor  yet  Pythagorean.  I  accompanied 
the  commencement  of  the  melody,  and  then  paused  wlule  the  singer  took  the 
Third  or  Sixth  of  the  key.  After  he  had  struck  it,  I  touched  on  the  instrument 
the  natural,  or  the  Pythagorean,  or  the  tempered  interval.  The  first  was  always 
in  unison  with  the  singer,  the  others  gave  shrill  beats. 

After  this  experience,  I  think  that  no  doubt  can  remain,  if  ever  any  doubt 
existed,  that  the  intervals  which  have  been  theoretically  -determined  in  the  preced- 
ing pages,  and  tliere  called  natural,  are  really  natv/ral  for  uncorrupted  ears  ;  that 
moreover  the  deviations  of  tempered  intonation  are  really  perceptible  and  un- 
pleasant to  uncorrupted  ewrs ;  and  lastly  that,  notwithstanding  the  delicate  dis^ 
tinctions  in  particular  intervals,  correct  singing  by  natural  intervals  is  much  easier 
than  singing  in  tempered  intonation.  The  complicated  calculation  of  intervals 
which  the  natural  scale  necessitates,  and  which  undoubtedly  much  increases  the 
f  manual  difficulty  of  performance  on  instruments  with  fixed  tones,  does  not  exist 
for  either  singer  or  violinist,  if  the  latter  only  lets  himself  be  guided  by  his  ear. 
For  in  the  natural  progression  of  correctly  modulated  music  they  have  always  and 
only  to  proceed  by  the  intervals  of  the  natural  diatonic  scale.  It  is  oidy  the 
theoretician  who  finds  the  calculation  complicated,  when  at  the  end  of  numerous 
such  progressions  he  sums  up  the  result,  and  compares  it  with  the  starting- 
point. 

That  the  natural  system  can  be  carried  out  by  singers,  is  proved  by  the  English 
Tonic  Sol-faists.  That  it  can  also  be  carried  out  on  bowed  instruments,  and  is 
really  carried  out  by  distinguished  players,  I  have  no  doubt  at  all  after  the  experi- 
ments of  Delezenne  already  mentioned  (p.  325,  note  *),  and  what  I  myself  heard 
when  I  was  hstening  to  the  violinist  who  accompanied  the  Enharmonic  Organ. 
Among  the  other  orchestral  instruments,  the  brass  instruments  naturally  play  in 
just  intonation,  and  can  only  be  forced  to  the  tempered  system  by  being  blown  out 
of  tune.*  The  wooden  instruments  could  have  their  tones  sUghtly  changed  so  as 
^  to  bring  them  into  tune  with  the  rest  Hence  I  do  not  think  that  the  difficulties 
of  the  natural  system  are  invincible.  On  the  contrary,  I  think  that  many  of  our 
best  musical  perJFormances  owe  their  beauty  to  an  unconscious  introduction  of  the 
natural  system,  and  that  we  should  oftener  enjoy  their  charms  if  that  system  were 
taught  pedagogically,  and  made  the  foundation  of  all  instruction  in  music,  in  place 
of  the  tempered  intonation  which  endeavours  to  prevent  the  human  voice  and 
bowed  instruments  from  developing  their  full  harmoniousness,  for  the  sake  of  not 
interfering  with  the  convenience  of  performers  on  the  pianoforte  and  the  organ. 

Musicians  have  contested,  in  a  very  dogmatic  manner,  the  correctness  of  the 
propositions  here  advanced.  I  do  not  doubt  for  a  moment  that  many  of  these 
antagonists  of  mine  really  perform  very  good  music,  because  their  ear  forces 
them  to  play  better  than  they  intended,  better  than  would  really  be  the  case  if  they 
actually  carried  out  the  regulations  of  the  school,  and  played  exactly  in  Pytha- 
gorean or  tempered  intonation.  On  the  other  hand,  it  is  generally  possible  to  con- 
vince oneself  from  their  very  writings,  that  these  writers  have  never  taken  the 
trouble  to  make  a  methodical  comparison  of  just  and  tempered  intonation.  I  can 
^  only  once  more  invite  them  to  hear,  before  uttering  judgments,  founded  on  an  im- 
pei^ect  school-theory,  concerning  matters  which  are  not  within  their  own  personal 
experience.  Those  who  have  no  time  for  such  observations,  should  at  any  rate 
glance  over  the  hterature  of  the  period  during  which  equal  temperament  was 
introduced.  When  the  organ  took  the  lead  among  musical  instruments  it  was 
not  yet  tempered.  And  the  pianoforte  is  doubtless  a  very  useful  instrument  for 
making  the  acquaintance  of  musical  literature,  or  for  domestic  amusement,  or  for 
accompanying  singers.  But  for  artistic  purposes  its  importance  is  not  such  as  to 
require  its  mechanism  to  be  made  the  basis  of  the  whole  system  of  music,  t 

*  [On  this  sentence  Mr.  Blaikley  observes  f  [This  last  paragraph,  from  *  Mnsidans 

(Proceedings  of  the  Musical  Association,  vol.  have   contested '    to  *  the   whole    system   of 

iv.  p.  56) :  '  It  seemed  to  me  worth  attention  mosic,'  is  an  addition  to  the  4th  Oerman  edi- 

that  this  must  be  talcen  as  being  particularly  tion.    The  remainder  of  this  Appendix,  whioh 

and  not  generally  true :  that  is,  that  though  concludes  the  work  in  the  3rd  German  edition, 

the  ideal  brass  instrument  has  such  character-  was  occupied  with  a  description  of  the  madeal 

istics,  this  ideal  is  not  necessarily  attained  to  notation  which  I  employed  in  my  footnotes 

in  practice.'   See  pp.  99  and  iQO,—Translator.]  and  Appendix  to  the  ist  English  edition ;  bnt 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


APP.  XIX. 


PLAN  OF  MR.   BOSANQUET'S  MANUAL. 


429 


APPENDIX  XIX. 

PLAN   OF   MR.    BOSANQUET'S   MANUAL. 

(See  p.  328c.) 

The  accompanying  figure  66  [taken  by  permission  from  p.  23  of  Mr.  R.  H.  M. 
Bosanquet's  Elementary  Treatise  on  Musical  Intervals  and  Temperament^  1^7 6] 

Pio.  66. 

Section 


\c 


Elevation 


cT^ch 


i^ 


\C 


_vg_ 


\y 


\d    ^ 


plan 


\c 


shews  the  arrangement  of  a  part  of  this  manual  for  53  equal  divisions  of  the 
octave.     The  upper  division  gives  a  longitudinal  section  of  two  digitals  standing 

as  I  have  long  since  abandoned  this  notation      p.  277,  I  have  thought  it  right  to  omit  the 
in  favour  of  that  introduced  in  Chapter  XIV.,      description.— TraTwZator.] 

Digitized  by  VjOOQ IC 


430  ADDITIONS  BY  THE   TRANSLATOR,  afp.  xix.  xx. 

one  above  the  other.  All  the  digitals  are  of  the  same  form  and  differ  only  in 
colour.  The  middle  part  of  the  figure  presents  an  elevation  of  the  front  ends  of 
these  digitals.  In  the  lower  part,  there  is  a  view  as  seen  from  above  [plan].  Pro- 
ceeding  firom  one  of  the  tones,  as  c,  upwards  and  backwards,  leaping  over  an  in- 
termediate digital  in  the  way,  we  pass  to  d  and  e  [on  white  digitals],  and  then 
continuing  by  major  Seconds  we  pass  tof%  ^,  c&  [on  black  digitals],  and  finally 
h^  or  /c  [on  a  white  digital  again].  *  The  sign  /  means,  as  has  been  explained 
in  the  text  (p.  329^),  sharpening  by  a  comma  \i  [or  one  of  the  53  degrees  =  2 2'642 
cents]  and  is  very  nearly  equivalent  to  our  superior  ^  [which  means  sharpening  by 
a  comma  of  Didymus  J^=2i*5  cents,  for  which  22  cents  is  usually  employed]. 
Between  the  members  of  this  series  are  inserted,  on  the  digitals  leapt  over,  those 
of  another  series  proceeding  by  major  Seconds,  d\^,  ^  [both  black],/,  g,  a,  h,  (!$ 
[all  five  white]. 

The  series  which  lie  just  above  one  another  differ  from  each  other  by  a  comma 
^  of  the  same  kind,  the  upper  being  ;khe  sharper. 

In  playing  the  scale  of  c  major  *  as  c,  d,  \  e,  /,  S^,  \  a,  \  b,  d,  observe  that 
a  horizontal  line  drawn  through  the  points  where  d  and  g  are  printed  in  the 
figure  will  just  pass  through  all  the  required  keys.  At  \  e,  \  a,  \  6,  we  thus 
come  on  a  deeper  intermediate  series.f  Every  major  scale  is  fingered  in  precisely 
the  same  way  no  matter  with  what  note  it  begins. 

The  harmonium  constructed  by  Mr.  Bosanquet  distributes  the  53  tones  over 
84  digitals,  some  of  those  at  the  upper  part  of  the  manual  being  identical  with 
some  of  those  at  the  lower  part,  in  order  to  avoid  having  frequently  to  jump  from 
upper  to  lower  digitals.  In  the  system  of  53  divisions  ///6  =  \(;,  since  five 
smallest  degrees  represent  a  diatonic  Semitone.  [For  a  further  account  of  Mr. 
Bosanquet's  notation  see  App.  XX.  sect.  A.  art.  27.  For  a  more  detailed  plan  of 
his  generalised  fingerboard  see  ihid,  sect.  F.  No.  8,  and  for  his  methods  of  tuning 
see  ibid,  sect,  G.  art  16.] 

*^*  \T}m  concludes  the  work  in  tJie  German.    Appendix  XX.  has  been  en- 
f  tirely  written  by  tJie  Translator j  and  Prof,  Helmholtz  is  in  no  respect  responsible 
for  its  contents.] 


APPENDIX  XX. 

ADDITIONS   BY  THE   TRANSLATOR. 
SECTION  A. 

ON   TEHPERAUENT. 

(See  notes  pp.  30,  281,  315,  and  329.) 

Art.  Art. 

1.  Object  of  Temperament,  p.  431.  12.  Generation  of  the  Metcatorial,  p.  432. 

2.  Equal  Temperament  and  Cents  defined,       13.  Notation  adopted  and  Fondamental  rela- 

p.  431.  tions  between   tempered  Fifths,  major 

3.  No  recarrenoe  possible  of  notes  tuned  by  Thirds,  Commas,  and  Skhismas,  p.  432. 

just  Fifths  and  just  major  Thirds,  p.  43  r .  14.  Linear  and  Cydlic  Temperaments,  p.  432. 

4.  Generation  of  the  Comma,  p.  431.  Linear  TemperamentSt  pp.  433-435. 

5.  Generation  of  Meantone  Fifths,  p.  431.  15.  The  Pythagorean,  with  its  asoal  27  notes 

6.  Generation  of  the  Skhisma,  p.  432.  tabulated,  p.  433. 

7.  Generation  of  Hebnholtzian  Fifths,  p.  432.  16.  The  Meantone  Temperament,  with  its  nsual 

8.  Generation    of    Skhismic   major   lairds,  27  notes  tabulated  and  their  pitch  nom* 

p.  432.  bers  calculated  for  4  pitches,  p.  433. 

9.  Generation  of  the  Pythagorean  Comma,       17.  The  Skhismic  Temperament,  p.  435. 

p.  432.  18.  The  Helmholtzian  Temperament,  p.  435. 

10.  Generation  of  the  equal  Fifth,  p.  432.  19.  Unequal  Temperaments,  p.  435. 

11.  Generation  of  the  Great  Diesis,  p.  432.  Cyclic  Temperaments,  pp.  435-441. 


*  [In  the    German   edition  a  cross  was  altered  accordingly.— TrofuZator.] 
placed  on  the  digitals  of  the  plan  which  were  f  [A    horizontal    line   through  6  in  the 

played  in  this  key,  but   the  German   copy  figure  will  pass  through  cdefgabc,  and 

could  not  be  used   here  because  it  followed  thus  give    the  Pythagorean    major  scale. - 

German  mnsical  notation.     The  text  has  been  Translator.] 

Digitized  by^OOQlC 


BBCT.  A.  ON  TEMPERAMENT.  43^ 

Art.  Art. 

20.  Conception  of  a  Cyclic  Temperament,  p.  Est^ve's  musician's  cycle  of  55;  in  ix. 

435.  as  8  :  5,  Henfling^s  cycle  of  50,  p.  436. 

21.  Equations  and  conditions  for  Cyclic  Tern-      24.  Paper  Cycles  for  calcodation,  x.  of  30103; 

perament,  p.  435.  xi.  of  3010;  xii.  of  301;  xiii.  of  1200, 

22.  Cycles,  i.  of  12  (equal);  ii.  of  31   (Huy-  P*  437. 

ghens) ;  iii.  and  iv.  of  53  (Mercator  and      25.  Equal  Temperament  tabulated  in  various 
Bosanquet),  p.  436.  pitches,  p.  437. 

23.  Cycles  derived  from  the  ratio  of  the  inter-      26.  Synonymity  of  Equal  Temperament  nota- 

vals  of  a  Tone  to  a  Semitone  in  i.  as  tion,  p.  438. 

2:1,  equal  cycle  of  12;  in  ii.  as  5  :  3,  27.  Notation  of  Bosanquet's  cycle  anditstabu- 

Huyghens^s  cycle  of  31 ;  in  iii.  as  9  :  4,  lation,  p.  438. 

Mercator*s  cycle  of  53 ;  in  v.  as  3  :  2,  28.  Expression  of  just  intonation  by  the  cycle 

Woolhouse's  cycle  of  19 ;  in  vi.  as  5  :  2,  of  1200,  p.  439. 

Chevy's  cycle  of  29;  in   vii.  as  7:4,  29.  Beferences,  p.  441. 

Sauveur's  cycle  of  43 ;  in  viii.  as  9  :  5, 

Art.  I. — The  object  of  temperament  (literally  *  tmiing  '),  is  to  render  possible 
the  expression  of  an  indefinite  number  of  intervals  by  means  of  a  limited  nmnber  ^ 
of  tones  without  distressing  the  ear  too  much  by  the  imperfections  of  the  conso- 
nances. The  general  practice  has  been  from  the  earliest  invention  of  the  key- 
board of  the  organ  to  the  present  day  to  make  12  notes  in  the  Octave  suffice.  This 
number  has  been  in  a  very  few  instances  increased  to  14, 16,  19,  and  even  to  31 
and  53,  but  such  instruments  have  never  come  into  general  use. 

Art.  2. — The  system  which  tuners  at  the  present  day  intend  to  follow,  though 
none  of  them  absolutely  succeed  in  so  doing  (see  infrk,  sect.  G.),  is  to  produce  12 
notes  reckoned  from  any  tone  exclusive  to  its  Octave  inclusive,  such  that  the  Octave 
should  be  just  and  the  interval  between  any  two  consecutive  notes,  that  is,  the 
ratio  of  their  pitch  numbers,  should  be  always  the  same.  This  is  known  as  Equal 
Temperament  (see  suprib,  pp.  320^  to  327c).  The  interval  between  any  two  notes 
is  an  Equal  Semitone,  and  its  ratio  is  i  :  *5/2=i  :  i •0594631,  or  very  nearly 
84  :  89.  If  we  farther  supposed  that  99  other  notes  were  introduced  so  as  to  make 
100  equal  intervals  between  each  pair  of  equal  notes,  these  intervals  would  be  those 
here  termed  Cents,  having  the  common  ratio  i  :  ^"^2=1  :  1*0005778,  or  very^ 
nearly  1730  :  173 1.  As  the  human  ear  is,  except  in  very  rare  cases,  insensible  to 
the  interval  of  a  cent,  we  need  not  divide  further,  except  occasionally  for  purely 
theoretical  purposes,  to  avoid  errors  of  accumulation,  as  in  this  section,  when  even 
the  thousandth  part  of  a  cent  may  have  to  be  dealt  with.  In  practice  the  errors 
of  tuning  would  soon  far  exceed  the  errors  arising  from  systematically  neglecting 
amounts  of  less  than  half  a  cent.  The  mode  of  finding  cents  from  the  ratios  of 
pitch  numbers,  wave  lengths,  or  vibrating  lengths,  is  given  in  sect.  C,  and  the 
values  of  most  of  the  usually  recognised  intervals  are  represented  in  cents  in 
sect.  D.    From  these  we  take,  up  to  3  places  of  decimals. 

One  just  Fifth  =7oi*955  cents 

„  „  major  Third  =386-314  „ 
„  „  Comma  =  21*506  „ 
„      „     Skhisma       =     1-954    „ 

Art.  3. — No  recurrence  of  notes  formed  by  taking  intervals  of  Fifths,  major  ^ 
Thirds  and  Octaves  is  possible  because  no  powers  of  the  numbers  7,  f ,  2,  or  of  any 
combination  of  them,  however  often  repeated,  can  produce  a  power  of  any  single 
one  of  them.  When  we  only  proceed  to  3  places  of  decimals  of  cents  (then  of 
course  using  multiples  for  powers),  there  would  be  a  recurrence,  but  so  remote  that 
it  would  be  practically  at  an  infinite  distance,  and  would  after  all  only  arise  from 
our  not  having  carried  the  decimals  fia.r  enough.  The  nearest  approximations  of 
any  practical  value  are  given  in  arts.  4,  6,  9,  11,  12. 

Art.  4. — Four  Fifths  up  and  two  Octaves  down,  together  with  a  major  Third 
down,  give  the  Comma  (of  Didymus  ratio,  80  :  81,  which  is  always  intended  when 
no  qualification  is  added),  that  is,  in  cents, 

4X7oi*95S  — 2x1200— 386'3i4=2i"5o6. 

Art.  5. — Consequently  if  we  used  Fifths  di-  precisely  two  Octaves  more  than  an  exact 

minished  by  the  small  but  sensible  interval  of  a  major  Third.    These  (for  a  reason  given  in 

quarter  of  a  Comma,  that  is  701*955 —}  x  21*506  art.  16)  were  called  meantone  Fifths,  and  were 

or  696-578  cents,  four  of  these  Fifths  would  be  long  in  use. 


Digitized  by 


Google 


432  ADDITIONS  BY   THE   TRANSLATOR.  app.  xx. 

Art.  6. — Eight  Fifths  up  and  also  a  major  Third  up,  with  five  Octaves  doinrn, 
give  the  Skhisma,  that  is,  in  cents, 

8  X7or955  +  386-3i4-5X  1200=1-954. 
From  this  we  can  deduce  two  different  usage& 

Art.  7.— If  we  employed  Fifths  diminished  by  a  Skhisma,  giving  386*314— 1'954«  384*360 

by  the    insensible    interval   of   i  x  1*954,  or  cents,  which  may  be  called  a  Skhlrauo  major 

701*955 -'24425-*  701-71075,    eight   of    these  Third,  then  this  major  Third  added  to  five 

Fifths  added  to  a  just  major  Third  would  give  Octaves  will  give  exactly  eight  Fifths.    This 

exactly  five  Octaves.    These  are  the  Fifths  is  the  relation  which  Prof.  Helmholtz  pointed 

proposed  supril,  p.  316a,  and  may  hence  be  out  (as  existing,  but  not  designed)  in  medieval 

called  Helmholtz's.  Arabic  scales,  supri,  p.  281a,  and  will  be  called 

Art.  8.— But  if  we  diminished  the  major  Third  Skhismic. 

Art  9. — Twelve  Fifths  up  and  seven  Octaves  down  give  the  sum  of  a  Comma 
1  and  a  Skhisma,  known  as  the  Pythagorean  Comma,  that  is,  in  cents, 

12x701-955— 7  xi2oo=23'46o=2i-5o6  + 1-954. 

Art.  10. — ^Hence  if  we  used  Fifths  dimin-  Skhisma,  which,  more   fully  calculated,  has 

ished  by  the  scarcely  perceptible  interval  of  i '95372  cents.    The  difference  between  these 

^x  23*460=  1*9550,    or    701-955-1*955  =  700  two  intervals  is  far  beyond  all  powers  of  ap- 

cents,  twelve  of  tiiese  Fifths,  known  as  equal  preciation  by  any  acoustical  contrivance.   The 

Fifths,   would    give    exactly  seven    Octaves.  Skhisma  will  therefore  be  considered  as  the 

These  are  the  Fifths  now  in  general  use.    The  twelfth  part  of  a  Pythagorean  Comma,  and 

amount  subtracted,  1*95500,  is  very  nearly  the  also  as  the  error  of  an  equal  Fifth.  See  p.  316^. 

Art.  II. — One  Octave  up  and  three  major  Thirds  down  give  the  difference 
between  two  Commas  and  a  Skhisma,  known  as  '  the  Great  Didsis,'  that  is,  in 
cents, 

1200-3  X  386 •3i4=:4i  059=2  X  2r5o6— 1-954. 

f  Art.  12. — Fifty- three  Fifths  up  and  thirty-one  Octaves  down  give  what  may  be 
called  a  Mercatorial,  because  on  it  depends  the  advantage  arising  from  the  use  of 
Mercator's  53  division  of  the  Octave.  It  is  less  than  two  Skhismas  by  about  one- 
third  of  a  cent,  that  is,  in  cents, 

53x701955-31  xi2oo=3-6i5=:2xi-954--293    (5) 

Consequently,  as  ^x  3-61 5= -068,  if  we  used  Fifths  which  were  too  flat  by  this 
imperceptible  interval,  or  had  701-955  — -068  =5701  887  cents  (which  may  be  called 
Mercator's  Fifths,  from  their  inventor),  we  should  have  precisely  53  Mercator's 
Fifths=3i  Octaves    (6) 

On  these  relations  depend  all  systems  of  temperament  which  are  worth  con- 
sideration. 

Art.  13. — Let  us  suppose  that,  measured  in  cents,  in  any  system  of  temperament, 

V  represents  the  Fifth  adopted,  T  the  major  Third  adopted,  and  K  and  S  the 

Comma  and  Skhisma  adopted,  so  that  j^-f  S  will  correspond  to  the  Pythagorean 

f  Comma,  and  2  K—S  to  the  Great  Didsis.    Then  as  the  four  first  relations  must 

hold  for  these  tempered  intervals,  and  an  Octave  has  1200  cents,  we  must  have 

from  art.  9;  12^-8400=  -iT-l-S,  whence  F=  700+ X  (Z'-hS) 

from    art.  11;  1200-31"    =2Z^-iS,  whence  T=4oo-^  (2J5r— 5) 

And  deducing  the  values  of  K  and  S  from  these  equations, 

ir=4F— T- 2400,  which  is  the  relation  in  art  4, 
;S=s8F+T— 6000,  which  is  the  relation  in  art  6. 

So  that  there  are  only  2  independent  equations  connecting  the  four  intervals 
F,  r,  K^  S.  Hence,  on  assuming  values  for  any  two  of  them  we  may  find  cor- 
responding values  for  the  other  two.  But  no  results  are  of  any  European  interest 
unless  F  and  T  both  approximate  very  closely  to  ihe  just  values  701*955  and 
386-314  cents. 

Art.  14.— There  are  two  quite  different  kinds  of  temperament,  the  Linear  and 
the  Cyclic.    The  Linear  contains  an  endless  series  of  notes  which  never  recur  in 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


SBGT.  A. 


ON  TEMPERAMENT. 


433 


pitch.  The  Cyclic  contains  also  an  endless  series  of  notes  which,  however,  do 
recur  in  pitch,  although  usually  under  different  names.  Hence  in  Cyclic  tempera- 
ments all  the  intervals  are  made  up  of  aliquot  parts  of  an  Octave,  or  1200  cents, 
which  is  not  the  case  in  Linear  temperaments.  In  both  of  them  the  main  object 
is  to  substitute  a  series  of  tempered  Fifths  for  the  several  series  of  Fifths  and 
major  Thirds  introduced,  supr&,  p.  276a,  and  exhibited  at  fall  in  the  Duodenarium, 
infr^,  sect.  E.  art.  18.  The  advantage  of  the  Cychc  over  the  Linear  temperaments 
consists  chiefly  in  a  power  of  endless  modulation — a  very  questionable  advantage 
when  harmoniousness  is  sacrificed  to  it. 


LiNEAB  Temperaments. 
Art.  15.— 27w  Pythagorean  or  Ancient  Greek  Temperament. 
Assume  F=7oi*955  and  J?=o, 

Then  from  art.  16,  5=  1 2  F— 8400=23*460 

and  T=  400  4-^.5=407 '820 

=386-3i4+2r5o6 


The  major  Third  is  a  whole  Comma  too 
sharp,  and  hence  this  system  is  quite  unfit  for 
harmony.  It  was  the  theoretical  Greek  scale, 
and  is  still  much  used  by  violinists.  See  Gomu 
and  Meroadier*s  experiments,  infr&,  sect.  G. 
art.  6  and  7.    The  following  are  the  27  tones 


which  this  temperament  would  require  for 
ordinary  modulations,  with  the  cents  in  the 
intervals  from  the  lowest  note,  the  logarithms 
of  those  interval  ratios,  and  the  pitch  numbers 
to  c'  264. 


PytJuigorean 

Intonation. 

No. 

I 

Note 

Cents 

Log 

Pitch 

No. 

Note 

Gents 

Log 

Pitch 

0 

0 

264-0 

15 

/« 

611-7 

15346 

375'9 

2 

b% 

23-5 

00589 

267-6 

16 

a'bb 

678-5 

1 702 1 

390-7 

3 

d'b 

90*2 

02263 

278-1 

17 

^ 

7020 

17609 

3960 

4 

cn 

1 137 

02852 

2819 

18 

m 

725-4 

18198 

401-4 

5 

e'bb 

180-5 

04527 

293-0 

19 

a'b 

792-2 

19873 

417-2 

6 

d' 

2039 

05115 

297-0 

20 

b'bb 

!J5-6 

20461 

422-9 

7 

c«« 

227-4 

05704 

3011 

21 

882-4 

22136 

439*5 

8 

e'b 

2941 

07379 

312-9 

22 

a' 

905-9 

22724 

445*5 

9 

d't 

317-6 

07967 

317-2 

23 

g'nn 

929-3 

23313 

4516 

10 

fy> 

384-4 

09642 

3296 

24 

i/b 

9961 

24988 

469-3 

II 

e 

407-8 

10231 

334-1 

25 

a'n 

1019-6 

25576 

475*7 

12 

f 

4980 

12494 

352-0 

26 

&b 

10863 
1109-8 

27251 

494-4 

13 

n 

5^J'5 

13082 

356-8 

27 

V 

27840 

501-2 

14 

l/b 

588-3 

14757 

370-8 

I' 

c" 

1200*0 

30103 

528-0 

IT 


These  can  be  all  exhibited  and  calculated  as  a  series  of  26  perfect  Fifths  up, 
namely, 

«t>t>  «bb  ^\>\>y  Jb  c\}  gb  clb  a\}  e\}  b\},  f  c  g  d  a  e  b, 

fi  4  dit  ^i^  <4  4  %  M<M  9U  ' 

The  17  notes  of  the  medieval  Arabic  scale  (supr^,  p.  281c')  are  those  in  the  first 
line  of  this  series,  adding  d\}\}  at  tlie  beginning,  and  omitting  b  at  the  end,  with 
all  the  j{l  and  jj^jj^  notes  in  the  Table. 

Art.  16. — The  Meantone  Temperament, 

The  major  Thirds  are  assumed  to  be  perfect,  and  the  Comma  is  left  out  of 
account.     Hence, 

T=386*3i4,  K=o.    Whence.by  art.  13 
7=696*578  as  in  art.  5,  and  S=— 41059. 


€k)n8equently  the  Second  of  the  scale,  which 
is  always  two  Fifths  less  an  Octave,  will 
have  193*157  cents  or  be  half  a  Comma  or 
10*753  cents  flatter  than  the  just  major  Tone 
of  203-910  cents  too  flat,  and  hence  by  the 
same  amount  sharper  than  the  just  minor 


Tone  of  182-404  cents.  From  this  mean  value 
of  the  Tofie  the  temperament  receives  its 
name.  This  was  the  temperament  which  pre- 
vailed all  over  the  Continent  and  in  England 
for  centuries,  and  for  this,  and  the  Pythagorean, 
our  musical  staff  notation  was  invented,  with 


Digitized  by  U'CfOgie 


434 


ADDITIONS  BY  THE  TRANSLATOR. 


APP.  XL 


a  distinct  difference  of  meaning  between  sharps 
and  flats,  although  that  difiFerence  was  different 
in  each  of  the  two  cases.  For  the  history  of 
its  invention  see  infri,  sect.  N.  No.  3.  This 
temperament  disappeared  from  pianofortes  in 
England  between  1840  and  1846.  (See  infr&, 
sect.  N.  No.  4.)  But  at  the  Great  Exhibition 
of  1 85 1  all  English  organs  were  thus  tuned. 
If  carried  out  to  27  notes  bearing  the  same 
names  as  in  art.  15,  but  having  the  different 
values  in  the  following  table,  it  would  pro- 
bably have  still  remained  in  use.  Handel  in 
his  Foundling  Hospital  organ  had  16  notes, 
tuned  from  dbtoat  in  the  series  of  Fifths  in 
art.  15.  Father  Smith  on  Durham  Cathedral 
and  the  Temple  organ  had  14  notes  from  ab  to 
dZ ,  and  the  modem  English  concertina  has 
the  same  compass  and  uses  the  same  tem- 
if  perament,  and  the  same  number  of  notes. 
The  only  objection  to  this  temperament  was 
that  the  organ-builders,  with  rare  exceptions, 
such  as  those  just  mentioned  (see  also  320c 
and  note  §),  used  only  12  notes  to  the  Octave, 
eb  bbf  eg  dae  bfZ  cU  gZ  •  The  consequence 
was  that  in  place  of  the  chords  ab  c  eb,  f  ab 
c,  <fec.,  organists  had  to  play  gUc  ebt  f  gZc, 
where  gU  was  a  Great  DiSsis  (41*059  cents)  too 
flat,  and  the  horrible  effect  was  familiarly 


compared  to  the  howling  of  '  wolves.*  Simi- 
larly for  bdZftit  was  necessary  to  use  b  eb 
/K ,  eb  being  a  Great  Di§sis  too  sharp,  with 
similar  excruciating  effects.  In  modem  mose 
it  is  quite  customary  to  use  keys  requiriog 
more  than  two  flats  and  three  sharps,  and 
hence  this  temperament  was  first  stjled  *  un- 
equal '  (whereas  the  organ,  not  the  Umpera- 
ment  was — ^not  unequsJ,  but — defeiUive)  sod 
then  abandoned.  But  with  the  27  notes  here 
given  there  would  have  been  nothiii^^  to  offend 
the  ears  of  Handel  and  Mozart.  At  the  pre- 
sent day,  ears  accustomed  to  the  sharp  lead- 
ing note  of  the  equal  temperament  (where  b  :  t! 
has  100  cents)  are  shocked  at  the  flat  leadisx 
note  of  the  meantone  temperament  when  b  :  (f 
has  117*1  cents.  But  played  with  27  or  36 
digitals  on  Mr.  Bosanquet's  generalised  key- 
board (Appendix  XIX.  and  also  XX.,  secL  F. 
No.  8)  it  is  the  only  temperament  suitable  to 
the  organ.  In  my  examination  of  50  tempos- 
men  ts  {Proc.  Boyal  Soc,,  vol.  xiii.  p.  404)  I 
found  tiiat  this  was  decidedly  the  best  for 
harmonic  purposes.  For  simple  melody  per- 
haps the  Pythagorean  is  preferred  by  violiniBte, 
but  that  was  always  absolutely  impossible  for 
harmony. 


Meantone  Intonation. 


No. 

Notes 

Cents 

Logs 

Handel 

Smart 

Helmholti 

I>aTham 

I 

& 

0 

0 

252*7 

259-1 

2640 

283-6 

'2 

c'U 

76-1 

01908 

264*1 

272*0 

275-9 

296-3 

3 

d'b 

117*1 

02938 

270*4 

277-3 

2825 

303-4      i 

4 

c'nt 

1 52*  I 

03816 

275-9 

282*9 

2882 

309-6      ! 

5 

d' 

1932 

04846 

2825 

2897 

295*2 

3171 

6 

e'bb 

234-2 

o«;876 

2893 

296-7 

302*2 

3247 

7 

d'% 

269*2 

06753 

295*2 

302*7 

308*4 

331-3 

8 

e'b 

310*3 

07783 

302*8 

3100 

315-8 

339-2 

9 

ef 

386*3 

09691 

315-9 
328*5 

3239 

330-0 

354-5 

ID 

/b 

427*4 

10721 

3317 

337-9 

363-0 

II 

e% 

462*4 

1 1599 

3301 

338-4 

344-8 

370-4 

12 

f 

503-4 

12629 

338-0 

346-6 

353-1 

379*2 

13 

ft 

579-5 

14537 

353-2 

362-1 

369-0 

396-3 

H 

l/b 

620*5 

15567 

361*7 

370-8 

377-8 

405-8 

15 

/«« 

655-5 

16444 

369-0 

378-4 

385-5 

414-1      ' 

16 

9" 

6966 

17474 

377-9 

387-5 

394-8 

424-0 

17 

a'bb 

737-6 

18504 

3870 

396-8 

404-2 

434-2 

18 

^n 

772*6 

19382 

394-9 

404-9 

412*5 

443-1 

19 

alb 

813-7 

20412 

404-3 

414-6 

422*4 

4537 

20 

nt 

848*7 

21290 

412*6 

423-0 

431-0 

463-0     1 

21 

a' 

8897 

22320 

422-5 

433-2 

441-4 

474-1      ' 

22 

i/bb 

9308 

23350 

432-6 

443-6 

452-0 

485-5 

23 

o'« 

965-8 

24228 

441-5 

4527 

461*2 

495-4 

24 

b'b 

1006*8 

25258 

452-1 

463-5 

4723 

507-3 

25 

b' 

1082*9 

27165 

472-4 

484-3 

493-5 

530-1 

26 

c'b 

1 1240 

28195 

483-7 

495-8 
508-4 

505-3 

542-8 

27 

6'« 

1158*9 

29073 

493-6 

515-6 
528*0 

553-9     1 

I' 

c" 

I200-0 

30103 

505-4 

518*2 

56r2 

On  account  of  the  great  historical  interest 
attaching  to  this  temperament,  I  give  the 
whole  27  notes,  shewing  their  value  in  cents 
and  logarithms,  whence  the  pitch  numbers 
can  be  calculated  out  for  any  pitch,  and  I 
have  actually  calculated  them  out  for  4  pitches. 
That  headed  'Handel'  has  A  422*5,  the 
pitch  of  Handel's  own  fork,  the  common 
pitch  of  Europe  for  two  centuries  (see  infr^, 
sect.  H.).  The  piano  of  the  London  Phil- 
harmonic Society  was  tuned  to  A  423*7  or 
very  nearly  this  pitch  when  that  Society  was 


founded  in  18 13.  But  about  1828  Sir  George 
Smart,  the  conductor  of  that  Society's  con- 
certs, raised  the  pitch  to  A  433*2,  as  I  have 
determined  from  his  own  fork,  and  oolumn 
*  Smart '  gives  the  notes  for  this  pitch.  As 
Sir  George  considered  the  fork  C  518  to  eorre- 
apond  to  his  A  433*2  (it  is  only  -2  vib.  too 
flat),  he  manifestly  used  meantone  temper*- 
ment  even  so  late  as  this,  for  the  equal  C  to 
A  433*2  would  have  been  much  flatter,  namely 
C  515*1.  This  was  a  very  curious  anticipa- 
tion of  the  French  pitch  of  1859.    The  next 


Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


SBCT.A.  ON  TEMPERAMENT.  435 

pitch  which  takes  Helmholtz*s  d  264  for  com-  sured  by  me.  Now  omitting  Smart's  pitch, 
parison,  gives  o!  441*4,  and  Father  Smith's  which  does  not  belong  to  the  old  organ  period, 
pitch  for  the  Hampton  Court  Palace  organ,  a  curious  relation  will  be  found  to  connect  the 
determined  from  an  unaltered  pipe,  1690,  was  other  three.  Handel's  dt  is  Helmholtz's  c', 
a!  4417.  Hence  this  was  a  regular  meantone  and  this  pitch  was  therefore  a  *  small  Semi- 
pitch.  The  last  column  shews  the  extra-  tone'  of  76*1  cents  sharper,  and  Handel's  d.' 
ordinarily  high  pitch  used  by  Father  Smith  282*5  is  practically  the  Durham  d  283*6,  which 
for  the  Durham  Cathedral  organ,  determined  was  therefore  a  Tone  of  193*2  cents  sharper 
from  an  original  ^8  pipe,  found  with  all  the  than  Handel's  pitch, 
others  by  the  organist  Dr.  Armes  and  mea- 

Art.  17. — The  Skhismic  Temperament. 

The  condition  is  that  the  Fifths  should  be  perfect  and  the  Skhisma  should  be 
disregarded.    This  gives 

F=7or'955,  S=o,  and  hence  by  art,  13,  ^=23*460, 

T=4oo— ?\Zr=384'36o=5386*3i4-- 1'954  as  in  art.  8.  m 
3 
That  is  the  major  Third  is  too  flat  by  a  of  Fifths  given  below  the  Table,  we  see  that 
Skhisma,  whence  the  name  of  the  tempera-  /b  is  the  eighth  Fifth  below  c,  which  follows 
znent  (see  supr&,  p.  281a).  The  effect  of  this  from  art.  13  whenever  S  ^  o.  Hence  gene- 
flat  major  Third  is  very  good  indeed.  On  rally  the  notes  in  the  top  line  will  be  major 
looking  at  the  Table  in  art.  15  we  see  that  c' l/b  Thirds  above  those  in  she  bottom  line  re- 
is  such  a  major  Third,  and  looking  to  the  lists  spectively — 

abb    ebb    bbb    fb    cb    gb    db     ab     eb    bb    f     c      g      d     a     e      b        ft       ct 
eb       bb      f        c      g      d      a      e       b      fl     ct    gt    d%    at    el     bt    fit    clt    git 

Having  an  English  concertina  (which  has  14  beats  16  times  in  10  seconds,  which  is  scarcely 

notes)  tuned  in  perfect  Fifths  from  gb  to  cS  in  perceptible  and  far  from  disagreeable.    But  it 

the  series  in  art.  15, 1  have  been  able  to  verify  is  evident  that  if  this  system  of  tuning  were 

this  result  for  six  of  the  major  Thirds,  and  to  adopted,  a  different  musical  notation  would 

determine  that  although  a  cl  e^  e  gl  &,  are  be  necessary,  and  a  convenient  typographical 

horrible  chords,  adbe^eab  b  are  quite  smooth  modification  of  Mr.  Bosanquet's  will  be  ex- 

and  pleasant.  The  major  Third  d  :fb  of  art.  15  plained  on  p.  438(2. 

Art.  18. — The  Helmholtzian  Temperament.  ^ 

In  this  case  the  major  Thirds  are  taken  perfect,  and  the  Skhisma  is  disregarded. 
Then  by  art.  13, 

K=i  2o'534=  2i'5o6  — 1*07^ 
and  7=70171  i=7oi*955—ix  i'954  as  in  art.  7. 

The  Comma  and  the  Fifth  are  therefore  imperceptibly  flattened.  In  this  case 
also  the  major  Third  would  be  the  eighth  Fifth  down.  And  the  same  reason  for 
altering  the  notation  would  hold  as  for  art.  17. 

Art.  19. — In  their  endeavours  to  avoid  the  *  wolves  '  of  meantone  temperament 
musicians  invented  numerous  really  unequal  temperaments,  which  it  would  be 
uncharitable  to  resuscitate.  There  is,  however,  a  really  practicable  unequal 
temperament  which  I  call  Unequally  Jv^t,  but  it  cannot  well  be  explained  till  the 
Duodenarium  has  been  developed.  (See  infr&,  sect.  E.  art.  25.)  I  proceed,  there- 
fore, to  the  consideration  of  the 

Cyclic  Tempebaments.  IF 

Art.  20.—  The  Octave  of  1 200  cents  being  divided  into  different  sets  of  aliquot 
parts  called  t^^ree^,  certain  numbers  of  those  degrees  may  approach  to  the  value  of 
the  just  Fifth  701-955  and  major  Third  386*314,  and  from  these  the  whole  scale 
may  be  constructed,  each  interval  being  more  or  less  well  represented  by  a  certain 
number  of  degrees.  There  would  then  be  this  advantage,  that  the  number  of 
values  of  notes  (whatever  happened  to  their  names)  would  be  strictly  limited  by  the 
number  of  degrees  in  the  Octave,  and  hence  values  would  recur,  and  the  whole 
scale  could  always  be  expressed  by  a  limited  number  of  cyclic  Fifths. 

Art.  21. — The  equations  for  finding  such  cycles  may  be  immediately  derived 
from  those  in  art.  13,  thus : 

Let  m  be  the  number  of  degrees,  and  ?^=:I20o-^m  be  the  number  of  cents  in 
one  degree,  so  that  i2oo=7wn.  Put  F=wt?,  T—nt,  K=nk,  S^^ns  in  the  four 
equations  art.  13,  and  divide  out  by  n.  Then  i2V— 7m=/:+«,  m— 3^=2^;— 5, 
whence  k^^v—t  -  2m,  5=8t;  +  ^— 5w.  On  assuming  values  for  m,  beginning  at 
12,  and  putting  first  A;=o,  and  then  5=1,  2,  3,  &c.,  or—i,  — 2,  — 3,  &c.,  and  next 

Digitized  by  VjDlDQlC 


43^ 


ADDITIONS  BY  THE  TRANSLATOR. 


APP.  XX. 


s=o,  and  then  ifc=i,  2,  3,  or— 1,-2,  — 3,  &c.,  we  get  the  corresponding  values  of 
V  and  t,  whence  the  scale.  Most  such  scales  would  be  useless.  For  practical 
tuning  m  should  be  small,  and  v  :  rriy  t :  m  should  be  nearly  the  ratios  of  the 
numbers  of  cents  in  the  just  Fifth  and  major  Third  to  1200  (or  of  the  logarithms 
of  their  interval  ratios  to  log  2='3oio3).    Now  the  approximate  values  of 


7or955 
1200 


are 


'-  -2' 


7 

— » 
12 


31 

53' 


&c.. 


and  of 


28        31      '59       53  ' 


1200 


are  -= 
3     12 


&c. 


Next  take  the  Pythagorean  major  Third  from  art    15. 
values  of 

3'   50        53 


The  approximate 


407 'Sio  I 

are  -» 

1200  2 


&c 


Finally,  take  the  meantone  Fifth  from  art  16.     The  approximate  values  of 

696-578  „,  ,    I     4    S    _9     14    23    . 

are  i,  ->    — »     >       »   — »   — >  ""^ 

1200     •""  *'  2     5     7     12     19    31 

Art  22. — These  numbers  suggest  cycles  of  12,  31,  and  53  degrees. 


i.  The  cycle  of  12  with  a  Fifth  of  7  and  a 
major  Third  of  4  degrees  would  imitate  Pytha- 
gorean intonation  well  and  just  intonation  in- 
difiFerently.  It  is  the  equal  temperament  of 
to-day.  Here  w«i2,  v  =  7,  ^  =  4,  k^s—o. 
In  cents,  one  degree  =100,  F=700,  r=4cx>, 
K=S  =  o.    See  art.  25. 

ii.  The  cycle  of  31,  with  a  Fifth  of  23  and 
«r  a  major  Third  of  10  degrees  would  imitate 
meantone  temperament  very  closely.  It  is  the 
Harmonic  Cycle  of  Huyghens.  Here  w=3i, 
t?=i8,  ^=10,  fc«=o,  a=i  — I.  In  cents,  one 
degree* 38710,  7« 696773,  T= 387-097,  J5:=o, 
§=-38710. 

iii.  The  cycle  of  53,  with  a  Fifth  of  31  and 
a  major  Third  of  18  degrees,  is  an  extremely 
close  approximation  to  Pythagorean  tempera- 
ment. It  is  Mercator's  cycle.  Here  m  =  53, 
v  =  3 1,  i  =  j8,  /c  =  o,  s  =  I.  In  cents,  one  degree 
» 22-642,  7=701-886,  !r  =  407-547,  ir=o, 
<S«  22*642. 

iv.  The  cycle,  of  53,  with  the  same  Fifth 
of  31,  but  a  major  Third  of  17  degrees  would 
give  a  sufficiently  dose  approximation  to  just 


intonation.  (See  p.  328<2.)  As  it  seems  to 
have  been  first  supplied  with  a  fingerboard 
by  Mr.  Bosanquet,  it  is  properly  called  Boean- 
quet's  cycle,  but,  as  will  be  seen  (infri,  sect.  F. 
No.  9),  Mr.  J.  Paul  White  has  also  invented 
a  keyboard  for  it.  Long  previously  to  either 
Gen.  T.  Perronet  Thompson  used  it  extensively 
in  his  works  on  the  Enharmonic  QttUar  and 
Just  JrUonation  as  a  convenient  approximation 
to  just  intonation,  and  from  his  works  it  was 
introduced  into  the  Tonic  Sol-fa  books  for  the 
same  purpose.  Here  m«53,  t?  =  3i,  <«I7, 
A;=.i,  8-0,  In  cents,  one  degree » 22*642, 
7=701-886,  7  =  384-905.  Observe  that  in 
art.  21,  Skhismic  7=701-955  is  only  the  im- 
perceptible interval  -069  cents  sharper,  and 
Skhismic  T=  384-360  is  only  the  imperceptible 
interval  -565  cents  flatter.  Hence  Skhismic 
intonation  and  Bosanquet 's  cycle  are  andibly 
interchangeable  within  the  limits  of  a  few 
keys.  It  is  only  when  the  modulation  beyond 
53  degrees  is  required  that  the*  cyclic  intona- 
tion has  the  advantage.    See  art.  27. 


Art  23. — ^Besides  these  the  following  temperaments  have  been  at  least  proposed. 
^  They  chiefly  depend  upon  assigning  imaginary  or  arbitrary  evaluations  of  the  ratio  of 
a  Tone  to  a  Semitone,  the  Comma  being  neglected,  and  the  Octave  supposed  to  con- 
sist of  5  Tones  and  2  Semitones,  so  that  if  Tone  :  Semitone  =p  :  q,  the  number  of 
degrees  of  the  cycle  will  be  5J?-f  27,  and  v^^p-^-q,  i=2p,  ^•=o,  2k— s,  the  Diesis 
=  — s  variable.  This  apphes  to  all  temperaments  where  K—Oy  thus  (art.  22)  in  i., 
p  :  g=2  :  I  ;  in  h.,  p  :  q=$  :  3  ;  in  iii.,  p  :  q—g  :  4. 


We  thus  obtain,  among  others, 

V.  Woolhouse's  cycle  of  19,  i> :  2 » 3  :  2, 
«»ii,i«6,  fc=o,  «  =  — i,2A;— fi«i.  In  cents, 
onedegree«63-i6,  F-694-76,  7=378-96. 

vi.  Chev6*8  cycle  of  29,  p  :  g-5  :  2,  t7:=i7, 
<ssio,  fc«o,  a-i,  2*— »==-!.  In  cents,  one 
degree «4i'38o.  7=703-460,  7=413-80. 

vii.  Sauveur's  cycle  of  43  m^rides,  p  :  q 
=  7  :  4,  v«25,  /=I4,  A;«o,  «=  -I,  2/c-s=  I. 
In    cents,    one   degree  =  27-907,    7=697-674, 

r«  390698. 

viii.  The  Musician's  cycle  of  55,  in  1755, 


according  to  Sauveur  and  E8t^ve,p  '  ff  =  9  :  5. 
u-32,  i  =  i8, /c  =  o,»=-i,2/c-s«i.  In  cents, 
one  degree  =21 -8 1 8,  7=698-176,  7=392*724. 

ix.  Henfling^s  cycle  of  50,  in  1710,  j?  :  g 
«8  :  5,  v«29,  <  =  i6,  A;=:o,  s=  — 2.  In  cents, 
one  degree  =  24,  7=696,  7=384,  JC«o, 
5* -48. 

Both  Fifth  and  Third  are  much  too  flat 
in  V.  and  too  sharp  in  vi.  Both  vii.  and  viii. 
were  decent  approximations,  and  convenient 
on  paper.  It  would  not  have  been  worth 
while  to  produce  them  on  instruments. 


Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


SECT.  A: 


ON  TEMPEBAMENT. 


437 


Art.  24. — But  paper  cycles  are  sometimes  extremely  useful  for  the  purposes  of 
calculation,  as  in  the  following  cases. 


X.  Cycle  of  30103  jots  (de  Morgan's  name), 
17  «  17609,  t^gSgi,  A;  =  539,  «  =  48,  2k-8 
=  1030.  In  cents,  one  degree  = '039863, 
y=  701-950,  r=  386-3135,  £-=  21-4862,  S^ 
1-91343.  , 

This  is  therefore  an  exceedingly  accurate 
representation  of  just  intonation.  It  is 
derived  from  looooxlog  2,  using  5  place 
logarithms,  and  the  only  reason  for  the  differ- 
ences from  just  intonation  is  that  it  is  taken 
strictly  to  the  nearest  integer.  Thus  loooo  x 
log  |s  17609*13,  and  this  would  have  made 
V  correct.  Mr.  John  Curwen  used  this  in  his 
Musical  Statics,  to  avoid  logarithms. 

xi.  Cycle  of  3010  degrees,  t;=  1761,  ^  =  969, 
fc  =  55»  s=»7,  2^—5  =  103.  In  cents,  one  de- 
gree  =  -3987i,  F«702-o6o,  ^=386-3135,  ^- 
21-9269,  5  =  2-7907.  This  is  derived  from 
1000  X  log  2  to  4  places,  and  is  consequently 
not  quite  so  accurate  as  the  last. 

xii.  Cycle  of  301  degrees,  ^=176,  ^  =  97, 
^  ="  5»  *  ■■  o,  2&  -  «  =  10.  In  cents,  one  degree  = 
3-9866,  7« 701-661,  r=386-7n,  2^-19-93355, 
iS«o. 

This  was  the  cycle  used  by  Sauveur  {Mim. 
de  VAcaditnie,  1 701,  p.  310)  as  a  finer  division 
than  was  given  by  his  cycle  of  43  m6rides  (see 
vii.).  As  301  a  7  X  43,  he  called  each  degree  a 
heptam6ride,  which  he  made  =-03987  of  an 
(equal)  Semitone.  He  also  gives  a  rule  for 
finding  the  number  of  heptamSrides  in  any 
interval  under  6:7a  267  cents,  which  is  the 
equivalent  of  my  rule  for  finding  cents  (infril, 
sect.  C,  I.  4,  note),  only  my  rule  extends  to 
498  cents.  Sauveur's  rule  is :  multiply  875  by 
the  differences  of  the  interval  numbers  and 


divide  by  their  sum.    For  6  :  7,  this  gives  67 

7 
heptam6rides,  and  as  log   z"'o67  to  three 

places,  this  is  correct.  It  is  the  earliest  in- 
stance I  have  met  with  of  the  bimodular 
method  of  finding  logarithms.  Sauveur's  875 
is  an  augmented  bimodulus  for  869,  for  the 
same  reason  as  I  selected  3477  in  place  of 
3462,  p.  447c'. 

I  have  here  taken  the  values  of  v  and  t  as 
they  ought  to  be,  but  judging  by  vii.  Sauveur 
took  t7--i75  and  £^98,  and  hence  got  the 
results  there  given,  which  agree  better  with 
meantone  intonation. 

Observe  that  the  Helmholtzian  7=701*711  ^ 
is  only  -050  cents,  or  imperceptibly  larger,  and 
the  cyclic  T  is  only  -397  cents,  also  imper- 
ceptibly larger  than  just.  Hence  the  Helm- 
holtzian intonation  and  Sauveur's  cycle  of  301 
are  interchangeable  within  301  degrees. 

xiii.  Cycle  of  1200,  or  the  Centesimal  Cycle. 
If  we  refrain  from  using  decimals  of  cents,  we 
really  use  a  cycle  where  one  degree » i  cent, 
17=:  7=702,  <=r=386,  A  =  ^=22,  a  =  5  =  2, 
2k—s-*^2.  These  are  quite  imperceptibly 
different  from  the  just  for  a  single  key,  but 
when  modulation  is  extended,  the  relative 
value  of  distant  notes  to  the  starting  note  will 
be  slightly  altered.  See  infr^,  art.  28.  In  the 
body  of  this  work  and  after  this  section 
*  cyclic  cents,'  as  they  may  be  called,  will  be 
used,  unless  accumulated  fractions  of  a  cent 
become  sensible.  But  in  the  investigation  of 
this  section  it  was  necessary  to  shew  differ-  f 
ences  much  more  minute  than  a  single  cent. 


Art.  25. — The  only  cycles  requiring  further  attention,  then,  are  i.,  the  Equal, 
and  iv.,  Bosanquet's.  The  method  of  tuning  Equal  Temperament  is  given  infr^, 
sect.  G.  art.  10.  The  pitch  of  the  notes  used  is  very  variable.  Six  principal 
pitches  are  tabulated  below. 

Eqtial  Intonation. 


Italian 

French 

Scheibler's 

Society 

English 

Schnitger, 

No. 

Notes 

Gents 

Logs 

Military 

Normal 

Stattgard 

of  Artfl 

Band 

1688 

i. 

ii. 

iii. 

iy. 

▼. 

Tl. 

I 

c' 

0 

0 

255-9 

258-6 

2616 

264-0 

26875 

290-9 

2 

c'« 

100 

02509 

2711 

274-0 

2772 

2797 

284-7 

3082 

3 

d' 

200 

05017 

287-3 

290-3 

293-7 

296-3 

301-7 

3264 

4 

d'n 

300 

07526 

304-3 

3076 

3iri 

314-0 

319-6 

3459 

5 

e' 

400 

I0034 

322-4 

3259 

3296 

332-6 

33!1 

366-5 

6 

/ 

500 

12543 

34>-6 

345-3 

349-2 

352-4 

3588 

388-3 

7 

^J 

600 

15052 

361-9 

36S-8 

370-0 

373*4 

380-1 

41 1-4 

8 

if 

700 

17560 

383-4 

387-6 

392-0 

395-5 

402-7 

435-8 

9 

^} 

800 

20069 

406-2 

410-6 

415-3 

419-1 

4266 

461-8 

10 

a' 

900 

22577 

430-4 

435-0 

4400 

444-0 

4520 

4892 

II 

a'5 

1000 

25086 

456-0 

4609 

466*2 

470-4 

478-9 

518-3 

12 

6' 

1 100 

27594 

483-1 

488-3 

493-9 

498-4 

507-4 

519J 

I' 

c" 

1200 

30103 

51 1-8 

517-2 

523-2 

528-0 

537-5 

581-8 

L  The  pitch  offioially  adopted  for  Italian 
military  bands  in  August  1884.  The  standard 
was  a  jBb456,  because  Bb  can  be  produced 
on  the  brass  instruments  without  using  the 
valves.  It  is  really  the  neai-est  approach  in 
whole  numbers  to  the  old  arithmetical  pitch 
of  C"5I2. 

ii.  French  diapason  normal  intentionally 


<k'435*  gl'^'ii^  eqnal  c"5i7*3i  is  practically  the 
same  as  Smart's  pitch,  which  is  the  lowest 
that  has  been  used  for  equal  temperament  in 
England,  and  was  contemporary  with  its  in- 
troduction there.  On  19  March  1885  this  was 
also  officially  adopted  as  the  pitch  of  Belgian 
military  bands,  which  had  hitherto  used  il45 1  *7, 
or  say  ^45 2,  as  given  in  col.  v. 

Digitized  by  V^OOQ IC 


438  ADDITIONS  BY  THE  TRANSLATOR.  Ai»p. «. 

iii.  Soheibler's    pitch,  proposed  at  Btutt-  t.  The  highest  asual  English  pitch,  known 

gardt,  often  called  the  German  pitch,  having  as  *  band  pitch,*  or  *  Kneller  HsJl  pitch,*  to 

^440.  which  military  brass  instruments  are  toned  in 

iv.  The  pitch  proposed  by  the  Society  of  England,  adopted  as  the  pitch  of  musical  in- 

Arts,  known  in  Germany  as  English  pitch,  struments  at  the  International  Inventions  and 

having  £"528,  and  a'444.    Unfortunately  the  Music  Exhibition,  London,  1885. 

original  fork  tuned  for  the  Society  of  Arts  by  vi.  The  original  pitch  of  the  St.  Jaoobi 

Griesbaoh  proved  to  be  c"534'46,  equivalent  to  organ  at  Hamburg  built  by  Schnitger  16S8, 

equal  a'449'4,  and  commercial  copies  vary  from  the  oldest  as  well  as  the  sharpest  example  of 

c''533*3  to  c''535'5.  equal  temperament  I  have  found.    On  these 

Helmholtz's  just  c"528  a'440,  may  be  con-  pitches,  see  the  abstract  of  my  History  cf 

sidered  as  represented  by  iii.  and  iv.  jointly.  Musical  Pitehj  infr4,  sect.  H. 

Ari  26. — The  notation  of  music  (see  art.  16)  was  adapted  to  either  the 
Pythagorean  or  Meantone  intonation,  in  which  there  was  a  Diesis  or  interval 
between  a  sharp  and  a  flat,  not  to  the  equal  where  the  Diesis  disappears  and  sharp 
coalesces  with  flat.  In  the  table  only  the  sharps  are  noted  as  usual,  but  they 
H  imply  flats.  If  we  arrange  the  notes  in  three  lines,  in  the  same  order  of  Fifths  as 
in  art.  15,  but  continue  them  to  36  tones,  we  shall  have 

F    C     O      DA      E     B     ik    CJjL  Q%     Dt    A% 

c»  ^  fM  <Mmt^^m^M  ftm  <Wt  mm 

The  middle  line  indicates  the  ordinary  notes,  but  those  in  the  upper  and  lower 
line  are  (at  least  from  a[>t>  to  ^'jf^  occasionally  met  with  in  modulations.  Now  the 
three  notes  in  each  column  have  the  same  meaning  precisely  in  equal  tempera- 
ment. They  are  absolutely  identical.  But  in  the  Pythagorean  and  Meantone  tem- 
peraments they  have  three  different  meanings,  as  shewn  in  the  tables  of  arts. 
15  and  16.  This  confusion  arises  from  equal  temperament  being  cyclic.  If  we 
begin  at  F  and  proceed  by  Fifths  to  A^  we  have  exhausted  all  our  12  valuea 
The  Fifth  above  A^  is  played  by  F^  but  it  would  be  considered  *  bad  spelling '  to 
write  it  so,  for  5|>  to  /  is  called  a  Fifth,  and  aj{l  to/ a  diminished  Sixth,  although  they 

^  make  the  same  interval  precisely.  This  arises  from  history.  It  has  been  pro- 
posed to  alter  the  notation,  but  the  objections  to  changing  are  so  great  that  the 
matter  is  mentioned  here  chiefly  to  explain  how  the  apparently  absurd  synonymity 
of  equal  temperament  arose,  and  also  because  this  synonymity  is  a  cardinal  point 
in  Mr.  Bosanquet's  notation. 

Art.  27. — ^In  Mr.  Bosanquet's  cycle,  art.  22,  iv.,  there  are  53  notes  to  be  supplied 
with  names,  and  moreover  after  the  53  notes  have  been  exhausted  the  values 
recur,  but,  if  the  old  notation  is  to  be  in  any  way  preserved,  the  old  names  do  not 
recur.  Hence  there  will  be  here  also  another  and  a  different  kind  of  synonymity, 
which  will  affect  the  position  of  the  notes.  In  the  following  tables  I  have  fii^ 
arranged  the  notes  by  Fifths  and  then  by  regular  ascent.  The  large  letters  may 
be  considered  regular.  They  are  each  supposed  to  have  all  the  synonyms  of  equal 
temperament  already  explained,  and  hence  are  written  only  as  the  line  of  capitals 
in  art.  26.  They  are  divided  into  '  sets  'of  12,  each  set  being  distinguished  by  a 
superior  or  inferior  number,  because  each  is  one  degree  (22*642  cents,  and  hence 
very  nearly  a  real  Comma  of  21*506  cents)  sharper  or  flatter  respectively.    Under 

%  these  capital  letters  is  written  the  number  of  the  note  in  the  cycle  according  to 
Mr.  Bosanquet's  arrangement,  dictated  by  practical  convenience  in  performance. 
Now  there  are  12  notes  in  each  line,  and  hence  after  4  lines  and  5  Fifths,  indicated 
by  II,  we  have  exhausted  all  the  53  values.  The  names  of  the  letters  are,  however, 
continued  on  the  same  plan,  but  they  are  now  synonymous  with  those  at  the 
beginning  of  the  series,  and  hence  the  name  of  the  54th  Fifth,  or  jP',  numbered 
27,  is  written  in  small  letters  under  the  first  note  FJ}^^  which  is  also  numbered 
27,  and  the  series  after  F^  in  capitals  is  written  after  /^  in  small  letters.  These 
small  letters  have  the  same  value  as  the  large  ones  above  them.  This  synonymity 
forms  the  chief  difiiculty  of  the  instrument  when  modulations  oblige  the  player 
to  proceed  beyond  the  first  53  notes.  For  this  reason,  partly,  Mr.  Bosanquet  has 
in  practice  extended  his  keyboard  to  contain  7  sets  or  84  notes.  The  inferior  and 
superior  numbers  are  my  typographical  contrivances,  not  Mr.  Bosanquet's.  He 
uses  sloping  lines  Uke  those  supr^,  p.  220c,  last  bar,  ascending  for  my  superiors, 
descending  for  my  inferiors,  and  repeated  twice,  three  times,  &c.,  for  my  2,  3, 
&c.    These  are  very  convenient  in  musical  notation,  and,  on  account  of  using  the 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


BCT.  A. 


ON  TEMPERAMENT. 


439 


qua!  temperament  synonyms,  are  the  only  alterations  required  to  adapt  ordinary 
dusic  for  performance  in  this  cycle. 


Jfr. 

BosanqueVs 

Notes-^n  Fifths. 

'■'.I 

c.« 

o,n 

I>Ji 

A^^ 

^. 

c. 

^. 

A 

A, 

E, 

B^ 

7 

5 

36 

14 

45 

23 

I 

32 

10 

41 

19 

50 

I 

c» 

^' 

d» 

a' 

6» 

6» 

f't 

c^n 

j7*« 

d-^ff 

a«5 

o«« 

c^ 

^Jt 

A« 

^Jt 

^, 

c. 

0, 

A 

A. 

^, 

^, 

^ 

6 

37 

15 

46 

24 

2 

33 

II 

42 

20 

SI 

3 

c* 

f 

d^ 

a» 

e» 

6« 

/*« 

c»« 

1^« 

df 

a«« 

^.« 

c.« 

^j» 

A« 

ilj 

^. 

c. 

(^t 

A 

^, 

-B, 

^1 

'9 

7 

3« 

16 

47 

25 

3 

34 

12 

43 

21 

52 

% 

c« 

IT* 

d» 

a» 

6« 

&• 

/'« 

c*Z 

g*ti 

d*« 

0^5 

>2 


8 


39 
17* 


17 


il5 
48 
a* 


F 
26 

6« 


C 

4 
6« 


35 


13 


44 
^5 


E 
22 


53    m 


R'lJ 

C'« 

G'« 

D'« 

m 

F» 

C» 

G» 

D» 

ii» 

E' 

-B» 

JI 

9 

40 

18 

49 

27 

1^ 

36 

14 

45 

23 

I 

!» 

c» 

g" 

d» 

a» 

e* 

n 

clJ 

g% 

d-« 

a«» 

10 


41 


19 
d* 


50 


28 


6 


37 


15 

ft 


46 


E* 

24 
d^5 


2 


Jtfr.  Bosanquet*s  Cycle  of  53. 


Cyc!Ic 

Names  and 
Synonyms 

Centa 

Logs 

Pitch 
Numbers 

Cyclic 

Names  and 
Synonyms 

Cents 

Logs 

Pitch 
Nombers 

I 

C.   6>  a«« 

1132-1 

28399 

507-69 

28 

l^C  ^ 

543-4 

13632 

36134 

2 

C,   6*  a'5 

"547 

28967 

514-37 

29 

F^tr 

566-0 

14200 

366-10 

3 

C,   6» 

1 1774 

29535 

521-14 

30 

l^  f' 

588-7 

14768 

370-92 

4 

C    b* 

I200»0 

0 

264-00 

31 

F'lP 

611-3 

15335 

375-80 

5 

C,«c»    6» 

22-6 

00568 

267-48 

32 

0.  nr 

6340 

15903 

380-75 

6 

cjrc«  &• 

45-3 

01 136 

27100 

33 

0,  />» 

6566 

16471 

385-76 

7 

C,Jc« 

679 

01704 

274-56 

34 

<?.  /*« 

679-2 

17039 

39084 

8 

C»  c* 

90*6 

02272 

278-18 

35 

9.  ^^^ 

701-9 

17607 

395-98 

9 

C'»c» 

113-2 

02840 

281-84 

36 

0,zg'   J*Z 

7245 

18175 

401-19 

10 

D.   c««c« 

135-8 
158-5 

03408 

285-55 

37 

G^g^  ft 

747-2 

18744 

40648 

II 

D,  c»» 

03976 

289-31 

38 

o,tg* 

7698 

19311 

411-83 

12 

D,  c*n 

i8i-i 

04544 

29312 

39 

Gt  g* 

792-5 

19879 

417-25 

13 

D  <n 

2038 

05112 

297-00 

40 

Q'tf 

8151 

20447 

422-74 

14 

D.«d»   c»5 

226-4 

05680 

300-89 

41 

A,  g'ng^ 

837-7 

21015 

42831 

15 

D^d"^   c'5 

249-1 

06248 

304-85 

42 

A,  ft 

860-4 

21583 

433-95 

16 

A«^' 

2717 

06816 

308-86 

43 

A,  ft 

883-0 

22151 

439*66 

17 

DJ  d* 

294-3 

07384 

312-93 

44 

A    ft 

905-7 

22719 

445*45 

18 

D'»d» 

317-0 

07952 

317-05 

45 

A,ta'  ft 

9283 

23287 

451-31 

19 

^,  d'^Jt^i* 

339-6 

08520 

321-22 

46 

A,ta^  ft 

950-9 

23855 

457-25 

20 

^,  d«« 

362-3 

09088 

325-45 

47 

A,ta* 

973-6 

24423 

4^3-27 

21 

E,   d^l 

384-9 

09656 

329-73 

48 

At  a' 

9962 

24991 

469-37 

22 

E    d't 

407-5 

10224 

334-07 

49 

A'ta> 

1018-9 

25559 

475-55 

23 

F,    6'    d'JJ 

4302 

10792 

338-47 

50 

J3,   a^ta* 

10415 

26127 

481-81 

24 

F,    6«    d'5 

4528 

11360 

34293 

5' 

3,  a't 

^"^ft 

26695 

488-15 

25 

F,   a» 

475-5 

11905 

347-44 

52 

B,   a't 

1086-8 

27263 

494-58 

26 

F      6« 

4981 

12496 

35201 

53 

B    a't 

1109-4 

27831 

501-09 

27 

Fjt/'      6* 

520-8 

13064 

35665 

i' 

B»   a««c. 

1132-1 

28399 

50709 

Art.  28. — The  cycle  of  1200  is  especially  used  for  indicating  the  relations  of 
just  notes  and  the  mode  in  which  the  notes  of  tempered  and  inharmonic  scales 
generally  fit  in  among  these  just  notes.  The  series  of  11 7  just  notes  to  the  Octave 
is  developed  in  sect.  E.  infr^,  and  the  value  of  each  note  is  given  {ibid.  art.  18)  by 
the  numbers  of  the  corresponding  note  in  the  cycles  of  53  and  1200.  The  number 
in  the  cycle  of  53,  by  means  of  the  table  in  art  27,  gives  all  the  information 
required  as  to  these  substitutes,  including  Mr.  Bosanquet's  names,  which  are 
different  from  those  assigned  to  just  intonation  on  the  principles  of  Chap.  XIV. 
pp.  2766  to  277a.  To  shew  the  difference  between  cyclic  and  just  cents  the 
following  table  is  added,  in  which  all  the  names  of  the  117  just  notes  in  the 

Digitized  by  V^OOQ IC 


440 


ADDITIONS  BY  THE  TRANSLATOB. 


APP.  IDL 


Doodenariimi  of  sect.  E.  p.  463,  are  placed  in  alphabetical  order  for  easy  reference, 
with  their  cyclic  and  just  cents,  logarithms,  and  pitch.  It  must  be  remembered 
that  the  inferior  and  superior  numbers  in  this  table  refer  to  differences  of  a  Comma 
of  22  cyclic  cents,  or  one  of  21-5  just  cents,  and  in  that  of  the  cycle  of  53  on  p.  439, 
to  a  degree  of  22*6  cents,  hence  the  same  name  has  distinctly  different  meanings. 
Thus  Bosanquet*s  No.  5,  or  C^fi^c^,  has  22-6  cents,  and  just  c^  has  21-5  cents, 
which  agrees  well  with  22*6,  but  just  C^fi  has  49*2  cents  and  agrees  more  nearly 
with  Bosanquet*B  No.  6,  or  Cjj^  with  45*3  cents. 

Expression  of  Just  Intonation  in  ths  Cycle  of  1200. 


Note 

Cyclic 
Cents 

Jnst 
Cents 

LogB 

Pitch 

A 

906 

9059 

22724 

445*5 

A^ 

928 

927-4 

23264 

451*1 

A, 

884 

8844 

22185 

440-0 

^» 

862 

862-9 

21645 

434*6 

A,t 

998 

998-0 

25037 

469-9 

A,l 

976 

976-5 

24497 

464*1 

A,t 

954 

955*1 

23958 

458-3 

A,tt 

1068 

I068-7 

26809 

480-4 

A.%t 

1046 

10472 

26270 

483-4 

A\> 

792 

7920 

19873 

417-2 

A'b 

814 

8137 

20412 

4224 

A^b 

836 

835-2 

20952 

427*7 

A^bb 

722 

721-5 

I8I00 

400-5 

A^bb 

744 

743-0 

18639 

405*5 

A*bb 

766 

764-5 

I9I79 

410*6 

A*bbb 

652 

650-8 

16327 
27840 

384*5 

B 

IIIO 

1109-8 

501-2 

"B. 

1088 

1088-3 

27300 

495-0 

5, 

1066 

I066-8 

26761 

488-9 

BS 

1 180 

ii8o'4 

29613 

522- 1 

B,% 

1 158 

1158-9 

29073 

515-6 

54 

1 136 

1 1374 

28534 

5093 

B,t% 

72 

72-5 

01822 

275*3 

B,l% 

50 

504 

01282 

271-8 

Bb 

996 

996-1 

24988 

469-3 

-B'b 

1018 

1017-6 

25527 

475*2 

B^b 

1040 

1039-1 

26067 

481-1 

B'bb 

904 

9039 

22675 

445.0 

B'bb 

926 

925-4 

23215 

450-6 

B»bb 

948 

946-9 

23754 

4562 

B<bb 

970 

968-4 

24294 

461-9 

B»bbb 

834 

833-2 

20902 

427-2 

jB'bbb 

856 

8547 

21442 

432-5 

C 

0 

0 

0 

2640 

C» 

22 

21-5 

00540 

267-3 

0, 

1178 

1 178-5 

29564 
02852 

521-5 

CI 

114 

"37 

281-9 

c,« 

92 

92-2 

02312 

278-4 

CA 

^2 

70-7 

01770 

275*0 

CJI 

48 

49-2 

01233 

271-6 

G^t 

184 

1844 

04625 

2937 

c.«« 

162 

162-9 

04085 

c,tt 

140 

141*3 

03546 

286*6 

c,tti 

254 

255*0 

06398 

305*9 

C'b 

1108 

1107-8 

27791 

5006 

C*b 

1 130 

4293 

28330 

5069 

C'b 

1152 

1150-0 

28870 

5'3*2 

C»bb 

1038 

1037-1 

26018 

480-7 

C»bb 

1060 

1058-7 

26557 

486-6 

C*bbb 

946 

945-0 

23705 

455*7 

D 

204 

203-9 

o5"S 

297-0 

D' 

226 

2254 

05655 

300-7 

A 

182 

182-4 

04576 

293*3 

A« 

296 

296-1 

07427 

313*2 

JD,« 

274 

2746 

06888 

3094 

A5 

252 

253*1 

06349 

305-6 

AS  8 

366 

366-8 

09201 

3263 

ajj 

344 

345*3 
458-9  ' 

08661 

3223 

A5«5 

458 

"513 

344- 1 

Kote 


Db 

2)»b 

2)«b 

JD'b 

D'bb 

D»bb 

2)*bb 

D*bbb 

E 

E, 

E, 

E,t 

E^t 

^,« 

E,t 

Eb 

E'b 

E^b 

E'bb 

E*bb 

E*bb 

E^bbb 

jB^bbb 

F 

Ft 
F,t 

Fjit 

F,tZ 

F,Zt 

F,Ztt 

F'b 

F'b 

F*b 

F'bb 

F*bb 

G 

G' 

G. 

G.« 

Ojl 

G,« 

(?,«« 

0,8  s 

o,tzt 

G'b 

Q'b 

(Pb 

G'bb 

0*bb 

G'bb 

G'bbb 


Cyclic 
Cents 


90 
112 

134 

156 

20 

42 

64 

1 150 

408 

386 

364 

5^ 
478 

456 

434 
570 

548 
294 

338 
224 
246 
268 
132 
154 
498 
520 
542 
476 
612 

568 
682 
660 
638 
752 
406 
428 
450 
336 
358 
702 

724 
680 

794 
772 
750 
886 
864 
842 
956 
610 
632 

654 
518 
540 
562 

448 


Just 
Cents 


90-2 
111-7 
132-5 
154-7 

19-6 

4I-I 
62-6 
1 148-9 
4078 
386-3 
364-8 
500-0 

478-5 
457-0 

435*5 
570-7 
549*2 
294-1 
3>5*6 
337*1 
223-5 
245-0 
266-5 

131-3 
1528 
498-0 
519*6 
54I-I 

476-5 
611-7 
590-2 
568-7 
682-4 
660*9 
6394 
753-1 
405-9 

427*4 
448-9 
335*2 
336*7 
702-0 

723*5 
680*4 

794*1 
772-6 

751*1 
886*3 
8648 

843-3 
957-0 
6098 

631*3 
652*8 
517-6 

i^ 

446*9 


Logs 


DiyiLized  by  v 


02263 
02803 
03342 
03882 
00490 
01030 
01569 
28821 
1023 1 
09691 
09152 
12542 
12003 
1 1464 
10924 
I4316 
13776 
07379 
07918 
08458 
05606 
06145 
06688 
03293 
03833 
12493 
13033 
13573 
1 1954 

15346 
14806 
14267 
17119 
16579 
16040 
18892 
I0181 
I0721 
1 1 261 
08409 
08948 
17609 
18149 
17070 
19922 
19382 
18843 
22234 
21694 

21 15s 
24007 

15297 
15836 
16376 
12984 
13524 
14063 
II2I0 


H^€- 


BBCT.B.     ON   THE   DETERMINATION   OF  PITCH  NUMBERS. 


441 


and  pitch  number  445*5.  Then  for  ^A,  Bosan- 
qaet'8  number  is  43  (having  883  cents,  log 
'22151,  pitch  number  4397)*  the  cyclic  cents 
are  906— 27  =  879,  the  just  cents  905-9-27'3 
=  878*6,  log  = '22724  — -00584  = '22140,  the 
pitch  number  =  445-5  -  A  x  445-5  =  445-5  -  6'96 
=  438*5,  shewing  that  Bosanquet's  substitute 
is  a  trifle  too  sharp. 


If  it  is  wished  to  introduce  the  series  of 
natural  harmonic  Sevenths  as  in  Poole  (infri, 
sect.  F.  No.  7),  X  being  any  note,  'X  will  have 
the  number  of  X  in  Bosanquet's  cycle  dimi- 
nished by  I,  the  cyclic  cents  of  X  diminished 
by  27,  the  just  cents  of  X  by  27-3,  the  log  of 
X  by  '00584,  and  the  pitch  number  of  X  by  ^ 
of  its  value.  Thus  A  has  Bosanquet^s  number 
44,  cyclic  cents  906,  just  cents  905*9,  log  -22724, 

Art.  29. — Those  who  require  more  information  are  referred  to  my  paper  on 
*  Temperament,'  Proc.  B.  S.,  vol.  xiii.  pp.  404-422  (where  the  subject  is  treated 
more  in  detail  and  in  an  entirely  different  manner),  and  to  the  memoirs  and 
essays  of  Salinas,  Zarlino,  Huyghens,  Sauveur,  Henflins,  K.  Smith,  Marpurg, 
Est^ve,  Cavallo,  Romieu,  Lambert,  T.  Young,  Eobison,  Farey,  Delezenne,  Wool- 
house,  De  Morgan,  Drobisch,  Naumann,  there  cited.  Also  to  Mr.  Bosanquet's 
treatise  on  Mtisical  Intervals  and  Temperament ,  1876,  to  his  papers  on  *  Tempera-  IF 
ment  *  in  the  Proc.  of  the  Musical  Association,  first  year,  pp.  4-17, 112-54,  and  his  ' 
article  on  *  Temperament '  in  Stainer  and  Barrett's  Dictionary  of  Mtisical  Terms, 
Also  to  Mr.  Lecky's  article  on  *  Temperament  *  in  Grove's  Dictionary  of  Music, 


SECTION  B. 

ON  THE  nSTEBlUNATION  OF  PITCH   NUUBEBfi. 

(See  notes  ppr.  11,  56,  168,  176.) 


No. 

I 


No. 

5. 
6. 


The  Clock  (Eoenig,  Lord  Bayleigh),  p.  442. 
Harmonium    Beeds   (Appunn,   Lord    Bay> 

leigh),  p.  443. 
Tuning-forks  (Scheibler),  how  to  form  and 

use  a  tuning-fork  tonometer,  p.  44.3. 


The  String  rEuler  and  Bernouilli,  Thomp- 
son, Griesoach,  Delezenne),  p.  441. 

2.  The  Siren,  p.  442. 

3.  Optical  Method  (McLeod  and  Clarke),  p.  442. 

4.  Electrical    Methods   (Mayer,    Glazebrook), 

p.  442. 

The  determination  of  the  pitch  number  of  any  note  heard  is  a  very  difficult 
problem  to  solve.     The  following  methods  have  been  used.  1[ 

I.  The  String,  Supposing  that  a  heavy  string  of  uniform  density,  perfect  elas- 
ticity, of  no  thickness,  but  capable  of  bearing  a  considerable  strain,  could  have  its 
vibrating  length  determined  with  perfect  accuracy — none  of  which  conditions 
can  be  more  than  roughly  fulfilled — then  the  pitch  numbers  of  its  parts  would  be 
inversely  proportional  to  their  lengths.  The  formula  has  been  worked  out  by 
Euler  and  Bernouilli,  and  amounts  to  this. 

Let  L  be  the  vibrating  length  of  a  suspended  string  in  English  inches,  I  the 
same  in  French  millimetres,  TFthe  stretching  weight  in  any  unit,  w  the  weight  of 
the  vibrating  length  of  the  string  in  the  same  unit,  V  the  pitch  number.     Then 

2  log  7  =  1-98485  +  log  Tr-(log  w  +  log  L) 
=  3*38968  +  log  W-{log  w  +  log  I). 

This  was  used  for  careful  measures  by  Euler,  Dr.  Eobert  Smith,  Marpurg, 
Fischer,  and  De  Prony.  Probably  on  account  of  the  necessary  thickness  of  the 
string  the  results  could  not  be  trusted  within  5  vib.  5I 


The  work  is  also  extremely  difiScnlt,  and 
depends  ultimately  on  determining  a  unison 
between  two  tones  of  very  different  qualities. 
General  T.  Perronet  Thompson  used  such  an 
instrument  for  tuning  his  organ  (described  in 
Just  Intanationt  7th  ed.  p.  69).  As  his  string 
was  No.  20  (i'i65  mm.  in  diam.)  no  reliance 
could  be  placed  on  the  perfect  exactness  of  his 
results.  Mr.  J.  H.  Griesbach  in  i860  tuned  a 
string  5*17  mm.  in  diam.  till  one  quarter 
of  its  length  was  in  unison  with  a  given  note, 
and  then  counted  the  yibrations  of  the  whole 
string  automatically.  The  instrument  is  in 
the  South  Kensington  Museum,  and  was  de- 
scribed in  the  Journal  of  the  Society  of  Arts, 
6  April  i860,  p.  353.  The  results  were  3  to  6 
vibrations  wrong. 

Delezenne  {M&m,  de  la  Soc.  des  Sciences  d 


-  Lille,  1854,  p.  i)  made  the  best  use  of  the 
string.  He  stretched  700  millimetres  of  wire 
on  a  violoncello  body,  and  tuned  it  to  Mar- 
loye's  128  vib.  (which  was  probably  accurate, 
as  Marloye*s  256  vib.  certainly  was),  and 
then  by  a  movable  bridge  cut  off  the  length, 
which  when  bowed  was  in  unison  with  the 
fork.  This  fork  had  been  adjusted  by  slid- 
ing weights  to  the  pitch  of  a  note  heard. 
Then  measuring  this  length  in  millimetres,  he 
divided  128  x  700^89,600  by  it,  to  find  the  vib. 
This  assumed  that  lengths  were  inversely  as 
the  vib.  or  pitch  numbers.  But  he  found  that 
the  same  fork  was  in  unison  with  203*8  mm. 
of  a  string  '6154  mm.  thick,  and  198*9  mm. 
of  a  wire  -1280  mm.  thick.  The  former  gave 
439*6,  the  latter  450*5  vib.  The  thick  wire 
therefore  gave  a  pitch  42  cents  flatter  than 


Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


442                         ADDITIONS  BY   THE   TEANSLATOR.  app.  xx. 

the  thin.    Hence  he  confined  his  obeervations  was  pressed  down  on  it  by  a  knife-edge.     The 

to  the  thinnest  wire  that  woald  bear  the  strain,  intervals  between  the  lowest  and  each  of  the 

In  conjunction  with  Mr.  Hipkins  of  Broad-  other  forks,  and  the  longest  and  each  of  the 

woods*  and  the  foreman  tuner,   Mr.  Hartan,  other  lengths  (assuming  them  to  be  invers^y 

I  made  some  experiments  on   2  April  1885  as  the  number  of  vibrations),  were  calculated 

with  the  monochord  at  Broadwoods*,  having  in  cents.    The  former  were  generally  sharper 

a  string  of  '98  mm.  in  thickness.    Nineteen  by  the  following  cents,  the  minus  sign  shew- 

tuning-forks  about  20  vib.  apart,  with  pitches  ing  when  they  were  flatter:  076133  9  ^^ 

from  22377  to  578-40  vib.  accurately  known,  2  23  22  91  118955-^-4.  These  irregular 

were  brought  into  unison  with  lengths  of  the  differences  demonstrate  that  such  a  monochord 

monochord  limited  by  a  movable  bridge  touch-  gives  very  uncertain  results,  even  when  the 

ing,  but  sliding  easily  under  the  string,  whioh  unisons  are  estimated  by  very  sensitive  < 


2.  The  Siren,  This  has  been  described  in  the  text,  p.  12.  But  only  the  most 
carefully  constructed  sirens  with  bellows  of  constant  pressure,  as  that  described  in 
App.  I.,  or  the  *  Soufflerie  de  precision '  of  M.  Cavaill6-CoU,  worked  by  well  prac- 
tised operators,  can  give  good  results.    Here  also  a  unison  between  tones  of  very 

^  different  qualities,  one  of  which  is  fixed  and  the  other  variable,  has  to  be  deter- 
mined. The  best  work  that  I  know  with  the  siren  was  that  done  by  M.  Lissajons 
(who  used  M.  Cavaill6-Coirs  bellows,  as  the  latter  tells  me)  in  determining  the 
pitch  of  the  '  Diapason  Normal '  at  Paris,  which  was  meant  to  give  435  vib.  at 
15®  C.=  59''  F.,  and  actually  gave  435'45  vib. 

3.  The  Optical  Method  of  Professor  Herbert  McLeod,  F.R.S.,  and  Lieut.  R.  G. 
Clarke,  E.E.  (Proceedings  ofBoyal  Society,  Jan.  1879,  vol.  xxviii.  p.  291,  BudPhilo- 
sophidtil  Transaction's,  vol.  clxxi.  pp.  1-14,  plates  i  to  3),  consisted  in  viewing  white 
lines  on  a  rotating  cylinder  through  the  shadow  of  a  vibrating  fork.  The  machine 
is  difficult  to  manipulate,  but  in  the  hands  of  its  inventors  gave  extremely  accurate 
results. 

4.  The  Electrographic  Method,  invented  by  Prof.  A.  Mayer,  of  Stevens  Institute, 
Hoboken,  New  Jersey,  U.S.,  consisted  in  causing  a  tuning-fork  by  means  of  a 
copper-foil  point  to  scribe  its  vibrations  on  the  camphor-smoked  paper  cover  of  a 
brass  rotating  cylinder,  and  marking  seconds  by  passing  a  strong  induction  spark 
through  the  fork,  scribing  point,  and  paper  cover  at  the  passage  of  a  seconds  pen- 

IT  dulum  through  a  spot  of  mercury,  and  then  counting  the  sinuosities  at  leisure. 
The  weight  of  the  scribing  point  had  to  be  allowed  for,  but  the  results  were  very 
accurate. 

For  another  means  of  determining  the  frequency  of  a  fork  used  as  an  inter- 
rupter of  electricity,  see  Mr.  B.  T.  Glazebrook's  paper  in  Philosophical  Magazine^ 
Aug.  1884,  vol.  xviii.  pp.  98-105. 

5.  The  Clock.  Dr.  Koenig  (in  Wiedemann's,  late  PuggendorflTs  Annals,  1880, 
pp.  394-417)  describes  a  large  tuning-fork  having  the  pitch  number  64,  which  was 
made  to  act  on  a  clock  at  a  constant  temperature  of  20°  C.=68**  F.,  functioning  as 
a  pendulum,  so  that  every  single  vibration  was  registered  for  many  hours,  and  he 
was  thus  enabled  to  determine  one  standard  pitch  with  extreme  accuracy.  He  also 
found  from  this  that  his  old  well-known  forks  of  nominally  256  double  vib.  had  been 
tuned  at  too  high  a  temperature,  and  at  20**  C.  gave  256'i774,  and  at  15**  C.=59**  F., 
256*28  d.  vib.  He  also  determined  the  pitch  of  the  Diapason  Normal  at  15^  G.  as 
435*45  d.  vib.    A  rise  in  temperature  of  1°  F.  flattens  tuning-forks  by  i  vib.  in 

f  from  16,000  (Eoenig)  or  20,000  (Scheibler)  or  22,000  (Mayer)  double  vibrations  in 
a  second  (see  my  '  Notes  of  Observations  on  Musical  Beats,'  Proc,  B,  S.,  28  May 
1880,  vol.  XXX.  p.  523),  and  flattens  harmonium  reeds  by  about  i  in  10,000  vib. 
(See  my  paper  *  On  the  Influence  of  Temperature  on  the  Musical  Pitch  of  Har- 
monium Reeds,'  Proc.  B.  S.,  Jan.  1881,  p.  413.) 

hordBAyleigji  (PhUosophical  Transactions,  placed  a  screen  perforated  by  a  Bomewhal 
1883,  Part  I.,  pp.  316-321)  describes  another  narrow  vertical  slit.  If  the  period  of  the  pen> 
method  of  determining  the  frequency  of  a  dulum  were  a  precise  multiple  of  that  of  the 
standard  fork  by  means  of  a  clock.  *  The  ob-  fork,  the  flash  of  light,  whioh  to  ordinary  ob- 
server looking  over  a  plate  carried  by  the  upper  servers  would  be  visible  at  each  passage, 
prong  of  the  fork  [of  intentionally  32  vib.]  ob-  would  either  be  visible,  or  be  obscured,  in  % 
tained  32  views  per  second,  i^.  64  views  of  permanent  manner.  If,  as  in  practice,  the 
the  pendulum,  in  one  complete  vibration.  The  coincidence  be  not  perfect,  the  flashes  appear 
immediate  subject  of  observation  is  a  silvered  and  disappear  in  a  regular  cycle,  whose  period 
bead  attached  to  the  bottom  of  the  pendulum,  is  the  time  in  which  the  fork  gains  (or  loses) 
upon  which  as  it  passes  the  position  of  equili-  one  complete  vibration.  This  period  can  be 
brium  the  light  of  a  para£Bn  lamp  is  concen-  determined  with  any  degree  of  precision  by  a 
trated.     Close  in  front  of  the  pendulum  is  suflicient  prolongation  of  the  observations.* 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


SECT.  B.     ON  THE   DETERMINATION  OF  PITCH  NUMBERS. 


443 


6.  Harmonium  Beeds, 

HeiT  Georg  Appunn,  of  Hanau,  invented  tonometers  of  65,  33,  and  57  reeds, 
serving  for  many  important  experiments  (see  for  example  supr^,  p.  176,  footnote  f). 
Copies  of  all  three  are  at  the  South  Kensington  Museum,  and  of  the  two  first  at  the 
Museum  of  King's  College,  London.  These  reeds  were  tuned  to  make  4  heats  in 
a  second  with  each  other,  so  that  the  65th  reed  made  with  the  first,  which  was 
an  Octave  flatter,  4x64=256  heats,  and  consequently,  according  to  the  theory  of 
beats  (see  text,  Chap.  VIU.),  the  pitch  numher  of  the  lowest  note  should  be  256. 
Unfortunately,  the  condensed  air  in  which  the  beats  took  place  accelerated  the  beats 
by  76  in  10,000,  as  I  determined  by  long-continued  observations  and  experiments 
(described  generally  in  my  first  paper  already  cited,  Proc,  B,  S.,  vol.  xxx.  pp.  527- 
532),  and  consequently  the  results  had  to  be  lessened  by  that  amount,  making  the 
pitch  number  of  the  lowest  reed  about  254.  These  reeds,  too,  were  not  sufficiently 
permanent  in  pitch.  Hence  this  instrument,  though  otherwise  very  useful,  failed  II 
in  determining  pitch  with  sufficient  accuracy. 


Lord  Bayleigh  (Proc  Mu$.  Aasn,  vol.  v., 
1878-9,  p.  15)  disoovered  a  way  of  determin- 
ing the  pitch  of  two  low  harmoniom  reeds, 
the  lowest  C  and  D  on  bis  harmonium.  Keep- 
ing the  wind  for  10  minutes  or  600  seconds 
as  constant  as  possible  and  using  resonators 
tuned  by  partially  covering  with  the  finger  to 
about  the  9th  and  10th  partials  of  the  low  C, 
two  observers  counted  the  beats,  one  between 
the  9th  partial  of  C  and  the  8th  of  D,  and  the 
other  between  the  loth  partial  of  C  and  the 
9ih  of  JD.    They  thus  found 

gC—  8  2)  =  2392+6oo 
9i>-io  0=» 2341 +600 

Whence 

C  =  (9  X  2392  +  8  X  2341)  ^600 
=  (21 528  +  18728) -r  600  =  67-09 

and  similarly 

i)  =  (io  X  2392  +  9  X  2341) -1-600= 74*98 


As  these  notes  make  an  interval  of  192*5 
cents  with  each  other,  Lord  Bayleigh  had 
evidently  (as  he  suggested)  altered  the  interval 
to  about  a  meantone  193*2  cents.  His  object 
was  to  determine  the  pitch  of  a  fork  of 
Koenig's,  supposed  to  vibrate  64  times  in  a 
second.  Now  as  Koenig's  256  is  really  256*28 
at  59^  F.,  this  64  should  be  64*07  at  the  same 
temperature.  Lord  Bayleigh,  to  take  ac- 
count of  the  effect  of  the  simultaneous  beating 
of  the  two  reeds,  sounded  both  of  them  at  the 
same  time  with  the  fork,  and  on  different  days 
obtained  the  following  results  (temperature 
not  named)  :— 


Harmonium  67*09 
67*04 

67*17 
67*19 


Tnning-fork  64*06 

>»    6407 

„         n    6417 

,>    6398 


Which  were  wonderfully  accurate. 


7.  Tuning-forks. 

All  the  above  methods  have  one  important  fault.  The  measuring  instruments 
are  not  easily  portable  and  not  readily  applicable  to  all  kinds  of  sustained  tones, 
while  the  two  first  required  trained  ears  to  discriminate  unisons  between  tones 
of  different  qualities,  with  great  accuracy,  that  is  to  say,  at  least  i  vib.  in  10  sec.  All 
these  faults  are  obviated  by  the  Tuning-fork  Tonometer  invented  by  J.  Heinrich 
Scheibler  (b.  1777,  e2.  1837),  a  silk  manufacturer  of  Crefeld  in  Ehenish  Prussia. 
The  simplest  process  of  making  such  a  Tonometer,  although  not  the  one  used  by 
Scheibler  (see  his  pamphlet  cited  in  note  t,  p.  iggd),  is  as  follows.  I  quote  prin- 
cipally from  my  '  Notes  on  Musical  Beats,'  already  cited. 


Obtain  a  set  of  about  70  good  forks  with 
parallel  prongs,  and  of  a  tolerably  large  size  ; 
tone  the  lowest  to  about  the  cf  (or  6  for  English 
high  pitch)  and  tune  the  rest  roughly  each 
about  four  beats  in  a  second  sharper  than  the 
preceding.  Then  fit  them  with  wooden  collars 
or  handles,  and  allow  them  to  rest  for  three 
months,  if  possible  in  the  same  temperature 
at  which  they  will  be  counted,  and  never 
alter  their  pitch  again  by  filing,  but  count 
the  beats  between  each  set  most  carefully,  at 
a  temperature  which  remains  as  uniform  as 
possible.  It  may  be  necessary  to  use  a  high 
temperature ;  thus  Scheibler's  was  from  15^  K. 
to  18°  R.« 65*75  to  72°-5  F.,  which  I  reckon 
at  69^  F.  as  a  mean ;  and  Koenig  now  works 
at  20°  G.  =s  68°  F.  Count  on  one  day  the  beats 
between  forks  i  and  2,  3  and  4,  5  and  6,  (fee, 
and  on  another  between  forks  2  and  3,  4  and  5, 


Ac,  so  that  the  same  fork  is  not  used  for  ^ 
two  counts  on  the  same  day.  Excite  by  strik> 
ing  with  a  soft  ball  of  fine  flannel  wound  round 
the  end  of  a  piece  of  whalebone,  as  a  bow  is 
not  convenient  unless  the  forks  are  tightly 
fixed.  Each  blow  or  bowing  heats,  and  hence 
flattens,  and  this  tells  if  the  experiments  on 
any  one  fork  are  long  continued.  Count  each 
set  of  beats  for  40  seconds  if  possible,  and 
many  times  over,  registering  the  temperature 
and  the  beats,  and  taking  the  mean.  Having 
counted  all,  observe  those  forks  which  are 
near  the  Octave  of  the  lowest  fork.  Find  two 
such,  beating  with  the  Octave  (that  is,  the 
second  partial)  of  the  lowest  fork  less  than 
they  beat  with  each  other.  Then  the  sum  of 
all  the  beats  from  the  lowest  fork  to  the  lower 
of  the  two  forks,  added  to  the  beats  of  the 
Octave  (that  is,  the  second  partial  tone  of  the 
Digitized  by  VjOOQlC 


444 


ADDITIONS  BY  THE   TRANSLATOR. 


APP.  XX. 


lower  fork)  with  that  fork,  is  the  pitch  of  the 
lowest  fork.  Hence  the  pitch  of  all  the  forks 
is  known.  The  extra  high  forks  are  for  verify- 
ing by  the  Octaves  of  several  low  forks,  and 
for  the  purpose  of  subsequently  measuring. 
From  such  a  tonometer  any  other  can  be 
made,  and  the  value  of  each  fork  at  another 
temperature  calculated. 

Scheibler  made  a  52-fork  tonometer  with 
infinite  trouble,  on  another  plan,  described  in 
his  book  and  counted  it  with  marvellous  accu- 
racy. This  tonometer,  which  I  have  made 
many  efforts  to  find,  has  absolutely  disap- 
peared and  his  family  knows  nothing  of  it. 
But  he  left  behind  him  a  56-fork  tonometer, 
believed  to  proceed  from  220  to  440  vibra- 
tions, by  steps  of  4  vibrations,  and  through 
the  kindness  of  Herr  Amels,  an  old  friend  of 

^  the  Scheibler  family,  who  obtained  it  from 
Scheibler 's  grandson,  I  had  the  use  of  it  for 
a  year.  I  had  to  count  it  as  well  as  I  could, 
just  as  if  it  had  been  a  set  of  forks  such  as  I 
have  described,  and  I  found  it  was  not  what 
was  thought,  but  that  only  32  sets  of  beats 
were  4  in  a  second,  and  the  other  23  sets 
varied  from  38  to  42  in  10  seconds.  I  found 
also  that  the  extremes  were  probably  of  the 
same  pitch  as  in  the  original  52-fork  tono- 
meter. After  then  counting  it  as  well  as  I 
could,  and  obtaining  219-27  vibrations  in  place 
of  219*67,  at  69°  F.,  I  distributed  the  error  of 
4  beats  in  10  seconds,  as  2  in  100  seconds, 
among  20  of  the  23  sets  which  were  not 
exactly  4  beats  in  10  seconds,  leaving  the 
first  3  sets,  which  I  had  repeatedly  counted 
and  felt  sure  of,  imaltered.  Then  I  reduced 
all  the  values  from  69**  to  59°  F.      Finally 

U  to  verify  my  result  I  measured  by  beats 
with  Scheibler's  forks  as  thus  determined; 
first  5  large  forks  of  various  pitches,  which 
I  had  had  made  for  me  in  Paris,  and  then 
4  forks  of  Eoenig's  belonging  to  Professor 
McLeod.  Professor  McLeod  himself  kindly 
measured  all  of  them,  also,  by  his  machine, 
and  Professor  Mayer  also  obligingly  measured 
the  first  5  forks  by  his  electrograpMc  method, 
both  with  the  greatest  care  and  precaution. 
The  three  sets  of  measurements  agreed  to 
less  than  i  beat  in  10  seconds,  and  more 
often  less  than  i  beat  in  20  seconds,  when 
reduced  to  the  same  temperature.  Thus  the 
value  of  the  tonometrioal  measurement  by 
beats  only,  and  the  possibility  of  counting. 
a  tonometer  sufficiently,  was  fully  esta- 
blished. Eoenig's  measurements  of  his  own 
forks  reduced  to  59^  F.,  and  of  the  actual 

%  Diapason  Normal  at  the  Ck)nservatoire,  Paris, 
intended  to  be  used  at  the  same  tempera- 
ture, also  agree  with  mine  within  less  than 
the  same  limits. 

The  pitch  of  all  sufficiently  sustained  tones 
can  thus  be  determined  mechanically.  We 
find  two  forks,  whose  pitch  is  known,  with 
each  of  which  the  new  tone  beats  more  slowly 
than  the  forks  beat  with  each  other,  and  we 
verify  the  count,  by  seeing  that  the  sum  of 
the  beats  with  both  forks  is  the  number  of 
beats  of  the  forks  with  one  another.  The 
pitch  number  is  then  that  of  the  lower  fork 
increased  by  the  number  of  beats  made  with 
it  in  a  second,  and  that  of  the  upper  fork 
diminished  by  the  number  of  beats  made 
with  it  in  a  second.  The  following  notes 
are  the  result  of  much  ezperienoe. 


Tuning-forks  are  comparatively  simple  in 
quality  of  tone  but  always  possess  an  audible 
second  partial  or  Octave,  and  sometimes  higher 
partials  still,  capable  of  being  so  reinforced 
by  resonance  jars  properly  tuned  to  them, 
that  beats  can  be  separately  obtained  from 
them  and  counted.  This,  as  we  have  seen,  is 
a  matter  of  great  importance  in  the  con- 
struction of  a  tuning-fork  tonometer.  When 
the  tone  is  very  compound,  as  in  the  ease  of 
bass  reeds  (especially  tliose  of  Appunn's  tono- 
meter, furnished  with  a  bellows  giving,  -when 
properly  managed,  a  perfectly  steady  blast 
for  an  indefinite  lengih  of  time),  beats  can 
be  obtained  and  counted  from  the  20th  to  the 
30th  and  even  the  40th  partial,  without  any  re* 
inforcement  by  a  resonance  jar.   (Seep.  I76c2'.) 

Taking  tuning-forks  first,  I  find  it  advan- 
tageous to  hold  the  beating  forks  over  one  or 
two  resonance  jars,  tuned,  by  pouring  in 
water,  to  the  pitch  of  the  partial  to  be  ob- 
served, whether  it  be  the  prime  of  both  or 
the  prime  of  one  and  the  second  partial  (or 
Octave)  of  the  other.  There  may  be  smaU 
differences,  but  I  have  not  found  any  diffei^ 
ence  appreciable  by  my  methods  of  observa- 
tion in  the  number  of  beats  in  a  second, 
whether  the  resonance  jar  is  the  same  or 
different  for  the  two  forks,  and  whether  it  is 
exactly  or  very  indifferently  tuned  to  each 
fork,  but  a  tolerably  accurate  tuning  mneh 
'  improves  the  tone  and  length  of  the  beat.  In 
that  case  the  resonance  jar  practically  quenches 
all  other  partial  tones,  and  the  beats  are 
distinctly  heard  as  loudnesses  separated  by 
silences.  If  no  jar  is  used,  the  other  par- 
tials are  heard.  In  the  case  of  the  Octave,  the 
low  prime  becomes  a  drone  and  fills  up  the 
silences.  In  the  case  of  beating  primes,  the 
Octaves,  which  are  beating  twice  as  fast,  tend 
to  confuse  the  ear.  Sometimes  the  second 
partial  of  a  fork  is  so  much  stronger  than  the 
prime,  that  when  the  fork  is  applied  to  a 
sounding-board,  only  the  Octave  is  heard, 
which  is  often  inconvenient  to  the  fork  tuner. 
This  is  entirely  avoided  by  the  resonance  jar. 
Beats  being  a  case  of  interference,  the  ampli- 
tude of  the  beating  partials  should  be  equa- 
lised as  much  as  possible.  With  two  forks  of 
very  different  size  and  power,  it  is  easy  to 
regulate  the  amplitude  by  holding  the  louder 
fork  further  from  the  jar.  Otherwise  the 
beats  become  blurred  and  indistinct.  For 
powerful  reeds  or  organ  pipes,  beating  with 
forks,  it  is  best  to  go  to  a  considerable  <lis» 
tance  from  the  reed  or  pipe  and  hold  the  fork 
close  to  the  ear  or  over  a  jar.  I  find  50  or  40 
feet  necessary  for  organs  ;  in  Durham  Cathe- 
dral, where  the  pressure  of  wind  was  strong 
and  my  forks  weak,  I  found  60  or  70  feet  dis- 
tance much  better.  As  I  was  not  iJ>le  latterly 
to  go  to  a  distance  from  Appunn's  reed  tono- 
meter, having  to  pump  it  myself,  I  foond  it 
impossible  to  count  the  primes  of  the  upper 
reeds  by  the  Octaves  of  my  forks,  which  were 
completely  drowned  by  the  reeds. 

I  find  beats  of  all  kinds  most  easy  la 
count  when  about  3  or  4  in  a  second.  They 
can  be  counted  well  from  2  to  5  in  a  second. 
Above  5  they  are  too  rapid  for  aconiacy; 
below  2,  and  certainly  below  i,  they  are  too 
slow,  so  that  it  is  extremely  difficult  to  teU 
from  what  part  of  the  swell  of  sound  the  bort 
should  be  reckoned.    Partly  from  this  reason. 


Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


EOT.  B. 


ON  THE  DETEBMINATION  OP  PITCH  NUMBERS. 


445 


erhaps,  I  have  found  great  variety  in  connt- 
ag  suooeBsiye  sets  of  such  slow  beats.  I 
lever  use  beats  of  less  than  one  in  a  second, 
I  I  can  avoid  it.  When  the  beats  are  slow  it 
9  difficult  to  discover  by  ear  which  of  the 
wo  beating  tones  is  the  sharper;  and  even 
Lne  ears  are  often  deceived.  It  is  easy  to 
liscover,  however,  by  putting  one  of  the  forks 
tnder  the  arm  for  a  minute.  This  heats  and 
lattens  it  by  2  or  3  beats  in  10  seconds. 
HLence  if  the  beats  with  the  heated  fork  are 
ilower,  that  fork  was  sharper,  because  it  has 
>een  brought  nearer  the  other ;  if  faster,  it 
ras  flatter  and  has  been  brought  further  away. 
>)unt  for  10,  20,  or  40  seconds,  according  to 
he  fork.  Up  to  20  or  30  beats  in  10  seconds 
t  is  easy  to  count  in  ones,  but  from  30  to  50 
t  is  best  to  count  in  twos,  as  one-ee,  two-ee, 
fee.,  beginning  with  one,  and  hence  throwing 
>ff  one  at  the  end.  When  counting  for  20 
seconds  I  always  count  in  twos,  and  for  40 
seconds  in  fours,  as  one-ee>ah-tee,  two-ee-ah- 
tee,  <ftc.,  because  I  have  to  divide  the  result  by 
zo  or  40 ;  and  this  division  is  avoided  by  the 
30unt  itself.  As  my  counting  was  never  for 
more  than  40  seconds,  small  errors  of  the 
olock  or  pendulum  were  imperceptible.  I 
B^enerally  used  a  marine  or  pocket  chrono- 
meter when  making  the  principal  count  for 
40  seconds,  but  for  merely  determining  pitch 
from  a  fork  of  known  pitch,  10  seconds  of 
time,  and  any  ordinary  seconds  watch  suffice. 
For  Prof.  McLeod's  observations,  which  lasted 
5  minutes  or  more,  extreme  accuracy  in  rating 
an  astronomical  clock  was  necessary.  Sup- 
pose a  watch  to  gain  5  minutes  or  300  seconds 
in  24  mean  hours,  which  is  an  extreme  case, 
80  that  86,700  watch  seconds  =  86,400  mean 
seconds,  then  10  watch  seconds  =  9*9654  mean 
seconds,  and  40  watch  seconds  =  39*8616  mean 
seconds.  Hence  no  perceptible  error  will  arise 
from  identifying  watch  and  mean  seconds. 
But  300  watch  seconds  =  298*962  mean  seconds, 
and  the  error  would  have  to  be  allowed  for. 
Scheibler  used  a  metronome  corrected  daily  by 
an  astronomical  clock,  and  graduated.  On 
this  a  movable  weight  enabled  him  to  make 
one  swing  of  the  pendulum  take  place  in  the 
same  time  as  4  beats  were  heard,  and  then 
from  the  graduation  he  read  off  the  rate. 
But  counting  by  the  seconds  hand  of  a  watch 
is  much  easier  and  altogether  more  conve- 
nient, while  it  is  probably  as  accurate.  Owing 
to  difficulties  in  beginning  and  ending  the 
count,  I  find  the  possible  error  per  second 
to  be  2  divided  by  the  number  of  seconds 
through  which  the  count  extends;  and  that 
it  is  best  to  take  a  mean  of  5  to  10  counts 
for  each  set  of  beats.  As  most  persons,  in- 
cluding myself,  begin  to  count  from  one  and 
not  from  nought,  it  must  be  remembered  that 
the  last  number  uttered  on  counting  the  last 
beat,  is  one  in  excess  of  the  real  number  of 
beats.  Thus  if  in  counting  for  10  seconds 
we  end  with  37,  the  number  of  beats  was  36 
in  10  seconds.  If  in  counting  by  twos,  one-ee, 
two-ee,  &G.,  the  last  was  19,  then  we  have 
counted  only  18  pairs,  and  hence  there  were 
also  36  beats  in  10  seconds.  If  we  end  with 
19-ee,  there  were  18^  pairs  or  37  beats  in  a 
second.  It  is  best  to  count  the  same  set  of 
beats  in  one,  twos  and  fours  to  realise  these 
corrections,  which  are  extremely  iiAportaftt 
Temperature   must    never   be   neglcNcted. 


Forks  should  not  be  touched  with  the  un- 
protected hand ;  they  otherwise  easily  flatten 
by  2  beats  in  10  seconds.  Interpose  folds  of 
paper.  I  use  two  folds  of  brown  paper 
stitched  between  two  pieces  of  wash-leather. 
Large  forks  are  generally  on  resonance  boxes 
and  need  not  be  touched,  otherwise  the  same 
precautions  should  be  used,  as  they  are  very 
sensitive,  and  retain  the  heat  longer  than 
small  forks.  Scheibler's  forks  are  fitted  with 
wooden  handles.  In  tuning,  the  file  heats  and 
flattens;  the  result  of  tuning,  therefore,  can 
seldom  be  known  for  a  day  or  two,  when  the 
forks  have  cooled  and  'settled,*  as  they  will 
be  sure  to  *  jump  up.*  I  find  it  best  to  leave 
off  filing  when  the  forks  are  two  or  three 
tenths  of  a  vibration  too  flat.  In  sharpening 
there  is,  therefore,  great  danger  of  doing  too 
much,  as  the  fork  remains  apparently  at  the  ^ 
same  pitch,  the  flattening  by  heat  balancing 
the  sharpening  by  filing.  Hence  all  copies 
should  be  compared  some  days  after,  by 
means  of  a  third  fork  about  four  vibrations 
flatter  or  sharper  than  each,  to  avoid  the 
slow  beats  of  approximate  unisons.  The 
filing  also  seems  to  interfere  with  the  mole- 
cular arrangement  of  the  forks. 

The  thermometer  should  be  always  con- 
sulted when  beats  are  taken.  But  if  the  beats 
are  between  two  forks,  of  which  the  pitch  of 
one  at  a  given  temperature  is  known,  and 
both  forks  may  be  assumed  to  be  altered  in 
the  same  ratio  by  heat,  then  the  temperature 
need  not  be  observed ;  but  the  unknown  fork 
may  be  presumed  to  be  as  many  vibrations 
sharper  (or  flatter)  than  the  measured  fork  at 
the  temperature  at  which  the  latter  was 
measured,  as  beats  in  a  second  were  observed  ^ 
to  take  place.  This  is  because  the  alteration 
is  very  small,  and  would  be  quite  inappreci- 
able for  the  few  vibrations  between  them. 
But  for  tonometrioal  purposes  an  allowance 
must  be  made. 

When  forks  are  counted  without  a  reson- 
ance jar,  they  should  not  be  applied  to  a 
sounding-board,  or  held  one  to  one  ear  and 
one  to  the  other,  but  should  both  be  held 
about  six  inches  from  the  same  ear,  and 
their  strengths  should  be  equalised  by  hold- 
ing the  weaker  fork  closer  to  the  ear  than 
the  stronger. 

When  the  forks  are  screwed  on  and  off  a 
sounding-board  or  resonance  box,  there  is 
great  danger  of  wrenching  the  prongs,  unless 
they  are  held  below  the  bend,  but  I  have  con- 
stantly seen  this  precaution  neglected.  A  cr 
wrench  immediately  affects  the  pitch  and 
duration  of  sound  of  a  fork,  and  renders 
it  comparatively  worthless.  Such  cases  have 
come  within  my  observation.  To  prevent 
wrenching  when  filing  forks,  only  one  prong 
should  be  inserted  in  the  vice.  And  for  even- 
ness file  the  same  quantity  off  the  inside  of 
each  prong,  counting  the  number  of  strokes 
with  the  file,  near  the  tips  for  sharpening, 
and  near  (not  at)  the  bend  for  flattening. 

The  next  enemy  to  be  guarded  against  is 
rust.  Forks  should  be  kept  dry,  and  occasion- 
ally oiled  with  gun-lock  oil.  Bust  towards  the 
tip  affects  the  fork  much  less  than  rust  at 
the  bend.  My  observations  and  experiments 
shew  that  errors  from  rust  can  scarcely  exceed 
a  flattening  of  i  vibration  in  250,  and  are 
generally  very  much  less.    But  as  the  amount 


Digitized  by 


^.joogle 


446 


ADDITIONS  BY  THE  TRANSLATOR. 


APP.  XX. 


is  uncertain,  rust  spoils  a  fork  for  accurate 
tonometrical  purposes.  With  care,  however, 
the  pitch  of  a  tuning-fork  remains  remarkably 
permanent.  Scheibler's  had  eyidently  not 
altered  in  more  than  forty  years. 

When  the  pitch  to  be  ascertained  is  of  a 
very  short  sounding  tone,  as  that  of  a  glass 
or  wood  harmonicon,  or  very  high,  it  requires 
an  extremely  delicate  ear  indeed  to  be  able  to 
determine  between  which  two  forks,  or  the 
octaves  of  which  two  forks  the  tone  lies. 
In  this  case  I  have  been  fortunate  in  having 
the  kind  assistance  of  Mr.  A.  J.  Hipkins  of 
Broadwoods',  without  whose  accurate  apprecia- 
tion of  differences  of  pitch  I  should  have 
frequently  been  altogether  at  a  loss. 

Dr.  Eoenig  (27  Quai  d'Anjou,  Paris)  makes 
^  tuning-fork  tonometers  of  beautiful  workman- 
ship, but  they  are  necessarily  very  expensive. 
The  largest,  proceeding  from  64  to  2 1 845  double 
vibrations  at  20*^  F.  with  sliding  weights,  costs 
1200Z.  The  medium  has  67  forks  from  256 
to  512  double  vibrations  at  intervals  of  four 
beats  each,  with  /  and  a!  which  fall  between 
pairs  of  forks,  mounted  on  resonance  boxes,  and 
costs  i2o2.  A  smaller  set  without  resonators 
costs  6o2.  A  small  set  of  13  forks  in  a  case, 
giving  the  equally  tempered  Octave  c'  to  c''  for 
a!  435  double  vibrations,  costs  only  *]l.  4^.,  and 
for  the  same  price  another  set  4  double  vibra- 
tions lower,  for  tuning  by  beats  to  French 
pitch,  can  be  obtained.    This  apparently  high 


price  arises  from  the  great  difficulty  of  taxiing 
such  a  succession  of  forks  with  perfect  acco- 
racy  to  particular  pitches.  This  accuracy  is, 
however,  not  necessary,  provided  the  coont  be 
accurate.  Any  tuning-fork  maker  would  make 
a  set  of  forks  such  as  has  been  described. 
The  count  must  be  made  by  the  investigator 
himself,  and  he  should  verify  by  a  set  of 
Eoenig*s  forks  of  c\  e',  ^,  c",  such  as  may  be 
found  in  many  places,  remembering  that  all 
the  older  sets  when  reduced  for  temperature 
give  at  15°  C.  =  59<»  F.,  d  256-3,  «'  320-3,  ^ 
384*4,  d'  512*6.  Prof.  McLeod*s  determina* 
tions  by  his  machine  at  this  temperature,  as 
the  mean  of  many  measurements,  were  c' 
256*310,  another  copy  256*306,  d  320-372,  tf 
3S4'437f  d'  512-351.  Eoenig  considered  them 
correct  at  26-2<*  C.  =  79*i6°  F. 

For  my  own  use  after  returning  Scheibler's 
forks  to  Mr.  Amels,  Ihad  105  forks  constructed 
proceeding  from  223-77  to  586*12  vib.,  which, 
after  being  tuned  partly  by  Scheibler's  maker 
in  Crefeld  by  the  forks  already  described,  and 
partly  constructed  to  differences  of  about  4 
beats  by  the  late  Mr.  Valantine  of  Sheffield, 
were  all  very  carefully  counted  again  by  me 
with  Scheibler's  own  forks,  the  means  of  manj 
determinations  up  to  two  places  of  decimals 
being  assumed  as  correct.  With  this  perfectly 
unique  set  of  forks  I  have,  since  that  time, 
made  all  the  determinations  of  pitch  mentioned 
in  this  book. 


ir 


Art. 
I. 


SECTION  0. 

THB  CAIiCUIATION  OF  CENTS  FROM  INTEBVAL  lUnOS. 

(See  notes  pp.  I3i  4I1  and  70.) 

Art. 


16 


Nature  of  Cents,  and  necessity  for  using 
them,  p.  446. 

I.  Firzi  Method,  without  Tables  or 
Logarithms^  p.  447. 

2.  When  the  interval    exceeds  an  Octave, 

p.  447. 

3.  When  the  interval  lies  between  a  Fourth 

and  Fifth,  p.  447. 

4.  When  the  interval  is  less  than  a  Fourth, 

p.  447. 

U.  Second  Method,  with  Tables  but 
without  Logarithms^  p.  447. 

5.  Previous  Reduction,  p.  447. 
IT    6,  7.  Rule  and  example,  p.  447-^» 

III.  Third  Method,  by  Five  Place  Logarithms, 
p.  448. 
8.  Reason  for  previous  methods,  p.  448. 

I.  When  the  '  interval  numbers,*  that  is  the  pitch  numbers  of  two  notes,  have 
been  found  (or  the '  interval  ratio,*  that  is  ratio  of  those  numbers  given  theoretically 
by  means  of  pitch  numbers,  or  of  numbers  in  proportion  to  them,  or  of  lengths  of 
strings  assumed  to  be  perfect,  or  of  wave-lengths),  it  is  necessary,  in  order  to  have 
a  proper  conception  of  the  interval  itself  by  comparison  with  a  piano  or  other 
instrument  tuned  in  intentionally  equal  temperament,  to  determine  the  number 
of  cents,  or  hundredtlis  of  an  equal  Semitone,  in  that  interval.  Such  cents  have 
been  extensively  used  in  the  notes,  and  occasionally  introduced  into  the  text,  of 
this  translation,  see  pp.  4id,  50a,  56a,  &c.,  and  supri,  sect  A.  art.  2,  p.  4316. 


9,  10.  Rule  and  examples,  p.  448. 

11.  To  find  to  the  tenth  of  a  cent,  p.  448. 

IV.  Fourth  Method,  by  Seven  Place 
Logarithms, 

12.  Rule  and  examples  finding  to  the  thoa^ 

sandth  of  a  cent,  p.  449. 

y.  Finding  Interval  Ratio  from  Cents. 

13.  Without  Logarithms,  p.  449. 

14.  By  Five  Place  Logarithms,  p.  449. 

15.  By  Seven  Place  Logarithms,  p.  450. 
Given  a  note  of  any  pitch  number  to  find 

the  pitch  number  of  a  note  which  makes 
with  it  an  interval  expressed  in  oenis^ 

p.  451. 
Principal  Table,  p.  450. 
AuxiUary  Tables,  I.  IL  IIL  IV.,  p.  45i» 


Digitized  by 


Google 


SECT.  O. 


ON  THE  CALCULATION  OP  CENTS. 


447 


I.  First  Method,  without  either  Tables  or  Logarithms* 

2.  If  the  greater  number  of  the  ratio  be  more  than  twice  the  smaller,  divide  the 
greater  (or  else  multiply  the  less)  by  2  mitil  the  greater  nmnber  is  not  more  than 
twice  the  smaller.  This  is  equivalent  to  lowering  the  higher  or  raising  the  lower 
tone  by  so  many  Octaves.  Hence  for  each  division  or  multiplication  by  2  add  1200 
to  the  result. 


Ex.  To  find  the  cents  in  47th  harmonic. 
Interval  ratio  i  :  47.  Multiplying  the  smaller 
number  5  times  by  2,  the  result  is  32  :  47,  for 


which  'reduced'  interval  ratio  we  have  to 
determine  the  cents  as  ander  and  then  add 
5  X  1200=^6000  to  the  result. 


3.  If  the  reduced  interval  ratio  be  such  that  3  times  the  larger  number  is 
greater  than  4  times  the  smaller,  but  twice  the  larger  number  is  less  than  3  times 
the  smaller  number,  then  multiply  the  larger  number  by  3,  and  the  smaller  by  4, 
for  a  new  interval  ratio,  and  add  498  cents  to  the  result.  m 


Ex.  For  32  :  47,  then  3  x  47  » 141  is  greater 
than  4  X  32  -  128,  but  2  x  47  »  94  is  less  than 
3  X  32 »  96.  Hence  we  use  the  interval  ratio 
128  :  141  and  add  498  cents  to  the  result.  If 
however  as  in  32  :  49,  twice  the  larger  number 
or  2x49^98,  is  not  less  than  3  times  the 
smaller  or  3  x  32  =  96,  we  use  this  interval 
ratio  96  :  98  or  its  equivalent  48  :  49  and  add 
702  cents  to  the  result.    In  the  first  case  the 

4.  Multiply  3477  by  the  difference  of  the  numbers  of  the  reduced  interval  ratio, 
and  divide  the  result  by  their  sum  to  the  nearest  whole  number,  and  if  the  quotient 
is  more  than  450  add  i.  To  the  result  add  the  numbers  of  cents  from  arta  i  and 
2.     The  result  is  correct  to  i  cent. 


given  interval  having  the  ratio  32  :  47  lies 
between  a  Fourth  and  a  Fifth,  in  the  second 
case  it  is  greater  than  a  Fifth,  but  in  both 
cases  the  reduced  interval  ratio  128  :  141  or 
48  :  49  is  less  than  a  Fourth.  The  object 
of  this  reduction,  which  is  seldom  necessary, 
is  to  have  to  deal  with  ratios  less  than  a 
Fourth. 


Ex.  I.  Interval  ratio  128  :  141 

13  di£Ferenc6 

10431 

3477 

Sam 

269)45201(168 

269      498    from  art.  3 

6000  from  art.  2 

1830    

1614    6666  cents  result 


2161    or  5  Octaves 
2152         6  Semitones 

and  }  Semitone 

in  the  interval  of  47th  harmonic  from  the  fun- 
damental. 

Ex.  2.  Interval  ratio  48  :  49,  difference  « i 

Sum 

97) 3477 (   36 
291     702 

567  738  centa  in  the  interval 
588  32  :  49,  or  about  Ji 
Semitones. 


♦»♦  The  number  3477  depends  on  the 
principles  explained  in  my  paper  *  On  an  C* 
Improved  Bimodular  Method  of  Computing 
Logarithms,'  Proc.  R.  5.,  Feb.  1881,  vol.  xxxi. 
p.  382.  Cents  are  in  fact  a  system  of  logs  in 
which  cent  log  2=1200,  and  its  bimodulus  is 
2  X  cent  log  2  -I-  nat  log  2-1 2400  -i-  '69315  » 
3462*4.  But  if  this  number  had  been  selected 
there  would  have  been  constant  additive  cor- 
rections from  the  first.  Hence  an  augmented 
bimodulus  3477  has  been  selected » 9  x  386^, 
or  9  times  the  cents  in  the  ratio  4:5.  The 
result  is  that  the  rule  is  exact  for  intervals  of 
a  major  Third.  For  less  intervals  it  gives  too 
great  a  result,  but  never  by  more  than  '6 
cent,  which  may  be  neglected.  Between  a 
major  Third  and  a  Fourth  it  gives  too  small 
a  result,  but  only  after  450  by  about  i  cent. 
For  small  numbers,  few  calculations,  and  in 
the  absence  of  tables  this  method  is  very 
convenient.  For  Sauveur's  previous  use  of  m 
an  augmented  bimodulus,  see  supri,  p.  4376, 
sect.  A.  art.  24,  xii. 


II.  Second  Method,  with  Tables  but  without  Logarithms, 

5.  The  reduction  for  Octaves  is  always  supposed  to  be  made  as  in  art.  2,  and 
hence  only  intervals  of  less  than  an  Octave  will  be  considered. 

If  the  interval  numbers  (art.  t)  contain 
decimals  they  must  be  multiplied  by  10  till 
the  decimals  disappear.     Thus  264  :  4785  is 

6.  Bule.  Annex  0000  to  the  larger  number  and  divide  by  the  smaller  to  the 
nearest  whole  number.     If  the  quotient  is  less  1 1290,  take  the  nearest '  quotient ' 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


taken  at  2640  :  4785,  so  that  the  rule  applies 
to  whole  numbers  only. 


448  ADDITIONS  BY  THE  TRANSLATOR.  -   app.  xx. 

in  the  Principal  Table  below.  The  corresponding  nearest  whole  number  of  *  cents ' 
is  on  the  same  line. 

Ex.  48  :  49.  12  I  490000    Nearest  quotient  in  Table  10210  for  oents  36  as 

• — in  art.  4,  ex.  2. 

Since  48  »  4  x  12  use  short  division.      4 1 40833 

10208 

7.  If  the  quotient  exceeds  11290,  look  for  the  next  less  quotient  in  Auxiliary 
Table  I.,  multiply  and  divide  by  the  numbers  in  the  col.  of  multipliers  and 
divisors,  and  thus  reduce  the  quotient  to  one  in  the  table.  Then  proce^  as  heiore 
and  add  the  number  of  cents  on  the  line  with  the  next  least  quotient  in  Aaxi- 
liary  Table  I. 

Ex.  Batio  32  :  47  as  in  art  3.    Since  32  »  4  x  8  proceed  thus : 
4  1470000 


81117500 


14688 

3    Next  less  quotient  13333,  mult.3,  div.  4,  oents  498. 


4:44064 


11016    Nearest  11019  cents  168 
add  498 

as  before,  666  cents  for  the  47th  harmonic. 

Actually  11016  lies  exactly  half-way  be-  the  error  of  i  cent  in  the  final  result  is  always 

tween  11013  and  11019,  and  in  that  case  the  possible,  and  is  here  disregarded.    It  is  avoided 

rule  is  to  tiJ^e  the  larger  number.    On  account  by  the  following  methods, 
of  the  approximatiye  nature  of  the  calculatioa 

fl  in.  Third  Method,  by  Five  Place  Logarithms, 

8.  This  is  by  far  the  best,  most  exact,  and  at  the  same  time  easiest  method, 
but  as  many  musicians  are  not  familiar  with  logarithms,  and  it  is  important  that 
they  should  be  able  to  reduce  interval  ratios  to  cents,  the  two  precedmg  methods 
have  been  inserted. 

9.  After  reducing  for  Octaves  as  in  art.  2,  subtract  the  log  of  the  smaller 
number  from  that  of  the  larger.  If  the  resulting  log  is  less  than  '05  268,  the 
number  of  cents  is  given  opposite  to  the  nearest  log  in  the  Principal  Table.  The 
decimal  point,  zeroes  after  it,  and  characteristic  are  omitted  in  practice  but  are 
used  here  for  completeness. 

Ex.  Interval  ratio  48  :  49.  The    difference    between   any   two   loga- 

log  49=1*69020  rithms   in  the  table  is  25  or  26,  hence  the 

log  48  a  1*68124  nearest  is  that  which  differs  by  less  than 

13.    In  this  case  it  differs  only  by  7. 

diff.       -00896 
m  nearest  log      '00903,  cents  36. 

10.  If  the  log  exceeds  -05268,  find  the  nearest  log,  in  Aux.  Table  11.,  subtract 
it,  take  the  cents  from  the  Principal  Table  for  this  diif.  as  in  art.  9,  and  add  the 
cents  opposite  the  next  least  log  in  Aux.  Table  11. 

Ex.  Interval  ratio  32  :  47. 

log  47- I '67210 
log  32=  1-50515 


ist  diff.    '16695 
Next  least,  Aux.  T.  II.     'i  5051,  cents  600 

2nd  diff.    '01644 
Nearest  log    '01656,  cents    66 

result  666,  as  before  for  the  47th  hannonic. 

zi.  If  it  is  desired  to  find  the  number  of  cents  to  the  nearest  tenth  of  a  cent. 

Digitized  by  V^OOQIC 


SEOT.  C. 


ON  THE  CALCULATION  OF  CENTS. 


449 


take  the  next  least  number  in  the  Principal  Table,  find  the  difference,  and  add  the 
tenths  of  cents  from  Aux.  Table  UI.    Thus — 

Ex.  in  art.  lo. 

2nd  diff.    -01644 
Next  least    '01631    cents  65 

3rd  diff.  13        >t         S 

cents  65-5  resalt. 

IV.  Fourth  Method,  by  Seven  Place  Logarithms. 

12.  As  a  general  rule  the  approximation  in  art.  10  is  amply  sufficient,  and  is 
generally  used  here.    But  occasionally  it  is  advisable  to  proceed  to  three  places  of 
decimals  of  cents,  as  in  the  whole  of  sect.  A.  on  *  Temperament.'    The  process  is 
then  conducted  by  Aux.  Table  IV.,  the  method  of  using  which  will  appear  by  the  ^ 
following  examples : — 

Ex.  I.  Interval  ratio  32  :  47. 

log  47=  1-672  0979 
log  32 -1-505  1500 


difference 
600     cents 

•166  9479 
•150  5150 

60       „ 

•016  4329 
•015  0515 

S                M 

•001  3814 
•001  2543 

•5        n 

•000  1271 
•000  1254 

•007   „ 

'000  0017 

665507  cents  result. 

Ex.  2.  Interval  ratio  264  :  478*5. 
log 4785  =  2679  8819 
log  264    «=  2-421  6039 

difference 
1000         cents 

•258  2780 
•250  8583 

20 

•007  4197 
•005  0172 

9 

•002  4025 
•002  2577 

•5 

•000  1448 
•000  1254 

•07       .. 

•000  0184 
•000  0176 

•007      „ 

•000  0018 

1029-5707  cents  result. 

V.  Method  of  finding  the  Interval  Batio  from  the  Cents. 

73.  Without  Logarithms.  If  the  cents  are  less  than  210,  the  ratio  is  that  of 
iocx)o  to  the  quotient  opposite  the  cents  in  the  Principal  Table.  If  the  cents  are 
greater  than  210,  subtract  the  next  least  number  of  cents  in  Aux.  Table  I.,  and 
multiply  and  divide  the  quotient  opposite  the  diff.  of  cents  in  the  Principal  Table 
by  the  corresponding  divisor  and  multipher  respectively  (observe  this  inversion, 
multiply  by  divisor,  and  divide  by  multiplier)  in  Aux.  TaUe  L 


Thus,  given  666  cents, 
Bubtr.  next  least  498  in  Aux.  Table  I.;  take  3  as 
—     div.  and  4  as  mult. 
168 

quo.  to  168  cts.  1 1019  in  Principal  Table. 
4 


This  is  the  correct  ratio  for  666  cents,  it 
is  larger  than  1*4688  obtained  from  32  :  47 
in  art.  7,  because  the  cents  were  in  excess,  IT 
but  the  difference  is  quite  unimportant. 


3  I 44076 

14692,  ratio  1*4692. 

14.  By  Five  Place  Logarithms. 

Given  665-5  ^^o^*  as  results  from  art.  11— 
Genta       hog% 
600        •15051 
65        •0163 I 
•5  -00013 


Aux.  Table  11. 
Principal  Table 
Aux.  TaUc  III. 


Bum  *  16695 » log  i*4^7i  which  is  now  'oooi  smaller  than  in 
art.  7,  and  is  the  correct  value  of  665*5  cents. 


Digitized  by 


^*bogle 


45® 


ADDITIONS  BY  THE  TBANSLATOR. 


APP.   XX. 


12. 


IS'  ^y  Seven  Place  Logarithms, 
From  Aux.  Table  IV.    Cents  665507,  art, 


600   cents 

•150  5150 

60 

•ois  0515 

5 

•001  2543 

•5 

•000  1254 

•007  „ 

*ooo  0018 

-which  is  the  more  correct  valtie  of  47-^32. 
obtained  by  carrying  the  diyision  one  alep 
further  than  in  art.  7. 


•166  9480  =  log  1-46875, 
Principal  Table  for  the  Calculation  of  Cents, 


Cents  Quotients  i  Logs 

Gents 

Quotients 

Logs 

Gents 

Quotients 
10638 

Logs 
02684 

Gents 
160 

Quotients 
10968 

1  I^8» 

I 

10006 

00025 

54 

IO317 

''111 

'°Z 

04014 

IF 

2 

12 

50 

55 

23 

108 

44 

709 

3 

17 

75 

56 

29 

405 

109 

SO 

734 

161 

10974 

04039 

4 

23 

OOIOO 

57 

35 

430 

no 

56 

760 

162 

81 

064 

5 

29 

125 

58 

41 

455 

163 

87 

089 

6 

35 

'5! 

59 

47 

III 

10662 

02785 

164 

94 

114 

7 

40 

176 

60 

53 

505 

112 

69 

810 

165 

.11000 

139 

8 

46 

201 

"3 

75 

835 

166 

06 

164  1 

9 

H 

226 

61 

10359 

01530 

114 

81 

860 

167 

13 

i«9  I 

10 

58 

251 

62 

65 

555 

"5 

87 

885 

168 

19 

214 

63 

71 

116 

93 

910 

169 

26 

240  1 

II 

10064 

00276 

64 

77 

605 

117 

99 

935 

170 

32 

265 

12 

69 

301 

65 

83 

63' 

118 

10705 

960 

13 

l^ 

326 

66 

89 

656 

119 

11 

985 

171 

11038 

04290 

14 

81 

351 

67 

95 

681 

120 

18 

03010 

172 

45 

315 

15 

87 

376 

68 

10401 

706 

173 

51 

340 

16 

93 

401 

69 

07 

731 

121 

10724 

03035 

174 

V 

365 

17 

99 

427 

70 

13 

756 

122 

30 

061 

175 

64 

390 

18 

10105 

452 

123 

37 

086 

176 

70 

415 

19 

10 

477 

71 

10419 

01781 

124 

43 

III 

177 

V" 

440 

IT 

20 

16 

502 

72 

25 

806 

125 

49 

136 

178 

!3 

46s 

73 

3» 

831 

126 

S5 

161 

179 

89 

490 

21 

10122 

00527 

74 

37 

II? 

127 

61 

186 

180 

96 

516 

22 

28 

552 

75 

43 

128 

67 

211 

23 

34 

577 

76 

49 

907 

129 

74 

236 

181 

11102 

04541 

24 

39 

602 

77 

55 

932 

130 

80 

261 

182 

09 

566 

25 

45 

627 

78 

61 

^ 

183 

15 

591 

26 

SI 

652 

79 

67 

131 

10786 

03286 

184 

22 

616 

27 

57 

677 

80 

73 

02007 

132 

92 

3" 

185 

28 

641 

28 

63 

702 

133 

99 

337 

186 

34 

666 

29 

69 

728 

81 

10479 

02032 

'34 

10805 

362 

'f^ 

41 

691 

30 

75 

753 

82 

85 

057 

>35 

II 

387 

188 

47 

716 

83 

91 

082 

136 

17 

412 

189 

53* 

741 

31 

10181 

00778 

84 

97 

107 

137 

23 

437 

190 

60 

766 

32 

87 

803 

85 

10503 

132 

138 

30 

462 

33 

93 

828 

86 

09 

157 
183 

139 

36 

487 

191 

11166 

04791 

34 

98 

853 

87 

15 

140 

43 

512 

192 

73 

816  1 

35 

10204 

878 

88 

21 

208 

193 

79 

842  1 

36 

10 

903 

89 

28 

233 

141 

10849 

03537 

194 

86 

867 

H 

H 

16 

928 

90 

34 

258 

142 

55 

562 

195 

92 

892 

38 

22 

953 

143 

61 

587 

196 

99 

917  , 

39 

28    978 

91 

10540 

02283 

144 

67 

612 

197 

11205 

942  1 

40 

33 

01003 

92 

46 

308 

145 

74 

637 

198 

12 

967  . 

93 

52 

333 

146 

So 

663 
688 

199 

18 

992  . 

41 

10240 

01029 

94 

58 

358 

147 

86 

200 

25 

05017 

42 

46 

054 

95 

64 

383 

148 

93 

713 

43 

S2 

079 

96 

70 

40S 

149 

99 

738 

20i 

11331 

05042 

44 

57 

104 

97 

76 

433 

150 

10906 

763 

202 

37 

067 

45 

63 

128 

98 

82 

458 

203 

44 

092 

46 

69 

154 

99 

88 

484 

151 

10912 

03788 

204 

50 

118 

47 

75 

179 

100 

95 

509 

152 

18 

813 

205 

57 

'12 

48 

81 

204 

153 

24 

838 

206 

64 

16S 

49 

87 

229 

lOI 

10601 

02534 

154 

30 

^l 

207 

70 

'93 

50 

93 

254 

102 

07 

S<9 

155 

37 

888 

208 

V 

218 

103 

13 

584 

156 

43 

913 

209 

83 

a 

SI 

10299 

01279 

104 

19 

609 

157 

49 

939 

aio 

90 

52 

10305 

305 

105 

25 

634 

158 

964 

1 
1 

1 

53 

II 

330 

106 

32 

659 

159 

62 

989 

1 

\ 

Digitized  by  V^OO^K^ 


SECT.  D. 


MUSICAL  INTERVALS. 


451 


16.  Hence  given  a  note  of  any  pitch  and  the  interval  in  cents  between  that  and 
uiother  note,  it  is  easy  to  determine  the  pitch  of  this  second  note. 


Ex.  Bequired  the  reduced  47th  harmonio  to 
A.  453*9,  the  concert  organ  pitch  of  Mr.  H. 
Willis,  to  which  is  tuned  the  great  organ  at 
ihe  Albert  Hall. 

log  4S3'9  =  2-65696 
Bents  665*5,  art.  14,  give  log=»  -16695 


log  666*7  =  2*8239 1 


Auxiliary  Table  I. 

Mnltiplien 

Gents 

Quotients 

and 
Divlsoni 

204 

1 1 250 

x8+   9 

3i6 

12000 

X5-5-   6 

386 

12500 

X4+    5 

498 

13333 

^3-5-  4 

702 

15000 

X2+    3 

884 

16667 

x6-rIO 

1018 

18000 

x5^  9 

1200 

20000 

X  I  -i-  2 

Aax.  Table 

Aux.  Table 

n. 

ni. 

Cente 

Logs 

Cents 

Log. 

100 

02509 

•I 

•00003 

200 

05017 

•2 

^ 

300 

07526 

•3 

400 

10034 

•4 

10 

500 

12543 

•5 

13 

600 

15051 

•6 

15 

700 

17560 

7 

18 

800 

20069 

•8 

20 

900 

22577 

•9 

23 

1000 

25086 

1*0 

25 

I  ICO 

27594 

1200 

30103 

Hence  666*7  ^^  ^^^  pitch  nomber  of  the 
note  required.  Thus  it  is  possible,  for  any 
given  pitch  of  the  tuning  note,  to  calculate  the 
pitch  of  the  notes  for  any  given  temperament, 
and  hence,  as  will  be  shewn,  to  tune  in  that 
temperament. 


Auxiliary  Table  IV. 

Cents 

Logs 

Cents 

Log- 

100 

025  0858 

•I 

000  0251 

200 

050  I7I7 

•2 

0502 

300 

075  2575 

•3 

0753 

400 

100  3433 

•4 

1003 

500 

125  4292 

•5 

1254 

600 

150  5150 

6 

1505 

700 

175  6008 

7 

•     1756 

800 

2006867 

•8 

2007 

900 

225  7725 

•9 

2258 

1000 

250  8583 

IIOO 

275  9442 

1200 

301  0300 

10 

002  5086 

•01 

000  0025 

20 

05  0172 

•02 

050 

30 

07  5258 

•03 

075 

40 

10  0343 

•04 

100 

50 

12  5429 

•05 

125 

60 

15  0515 

06 

»5i 

70 

17  5601 

•07 

176 

80 

20  0687 

•08 

201 

90 

22  5773 

09 

226 

I 

000  2509 

•001 

000  0003 

2 

5017 

*002 

1 

3 

7526 

•003 

4 

001  0034 

•004 

000  0010 

5 

2543 

•005 

13 

6 

5052 

•006 

15 

7 

7560 

•007 

18 

8 

002  0069 

008 

20 

9 

2577 

•009 

23 

SECTION  D. 

MUSICAL  INTEBVALS,  NOT  EXCEXDINO  AN  OCTAVE,  ARBAKOED  IN  ORDER  OF  WIDTH. 

(See  notes  pp.  13,  187,  213,  264,  and  333.) 


IT 


I. 
t. 


Width  of  an  interval,  p.  451. 

Cyclic  and  actual  Fifths  and  major  Thirds, 

p.  452. 
{.  Cyclic  and  Exact  cents,  p.  452. 
|.  Interval  ratio,  p.  452. 
^.  £x>garithms,  p.  452. 
J.  Theoretical  and  practical  intervals,  p.  452. 
^  Calculation  of  just  intervals  by  Fifths  and 

major  Thirds  up  and  down,  p.  452. 
$.  Calculation  of  interval  ratios  in  the  same 

way,  p.  452. 


Art. 

9.  Harmonics,  p.  452. 
10.  Intervals  in  an  Octave,  p.  453. 

Table  I.  Intervals  not  exceeding  an  Octave, 

p.  453.     , 
Table  U.  The  unevenly  numbered  harmo- 
nics of  C7  66  up  to  the  63rd,  p.  457. 
Table  III.  Number  of  any  interval,  not  ex- 
ceeding the  Tritone,  contained  in  an 
Octave,  p.  457. 


I .  An  interval  was  defined  supr^,  p.  i  ^d,  note  J.  The  tindth  of  an  interval  is 
(neasiired  by  the  number  of  cents  it  contains.  Beside  the  usual  diatonic  intervals, 
a,  large  number  of  others  occasionally  occur,  which  it  is  convenient  to  have  arranged 

Digitized  by^eOgle 


452 


ADDITIONS  BY  THE   TRANSLATOR. 


APP.  XX. 


according  to  their  widths  as  measured  in  cents, 
the  following  table. 


Many  of  these  are  famished  in 


2.  The  oents  used  are  cyclic  cents,  as  de- 
fined sapri,  sect.  A.  art.  24,  xiii.  p.  4376',  that  is, 
those  intervals  found  by  taking  a  certain  num- 
ber of  Fifths  and  major  Thirds  up  and  down 
and  reducing  the  result  to  the  same  octave, 
are  assumed  to  have  cyclic  Fifths  and  major 
Thirds  of  702  and  386  cents  respectively.  But 
as  the  actual  Fifths  and  major  Thirds  have 
70''955  ^^^  3^6*314  cents  respectively,  a  slight 
error  in  excess  is  made  in  every  Fifth  up  and 
every  major  Third  down,  and  in  defect  in  every 
Fifth  down  and  major  Third  up,  which,  when 
a  great  many  are  supposed  to  be  taken  for 
theoretical  purposes,  may  reach  to  a  sensible 

5[  amount.  These  errors  are  of  no  consequence 
for  ordinary  purposes,  but  a  means  of  correct- 
ing them  is  given  in  sect.  E.,  p.  463d,  and  here 
it  has  been  tiiought  better  to  add  the  result  to 
three  places  of  decimals  in  many  cases,  and 
this  is  put  in  the  last  column,  preceded  by  the 
letters  *  ex.,*  meaning  *  more  exact  cents.' 

3.  Other  intervals  are  given  to  the  nearest 
whole  number  of  cents,  determined  as  in  sect. 
C,  which  therefore  belong  to  the  cycle  of  1200, 
and  hence  are  rightly  called  cyclic.  Here  also 
is  added  the  result  to  three  places  of  decimals, 
when  it  appeared  advisable  for  theoretical 
purposes.  For  ordinary  purposes  cyclic  oents 
always  suffice. 

4.  The  interval  ratios^  being  of  historical 


interest,  are  always  given,  although  they  are 
of  no  assistance  to  the  eye  in  recognising  the 
width  of  an  interval.  In  these  ratios  the 
smaller  number  is  always  placed  first.  In  the 
case  of  tempered  intervals  an  approximate 
ratio,  with  f  prefixed,  is  given  in  the  second 
column,  and  the  *  ex.*  or  more  exact '  ratio'  it 
given  in  the  last  column. 

5.  The  (five  place)  logarithm  of  each  inter- 
val ratio,  considered  as  a  fraction  of  which  the 
larger  number  is  the  numerator,  is  added  in 
each  case,  to  enable  those  who  understand 
logarithms  to  deal  with  them  inmiediately  in 
calculating  pitch  numbers,  Ac.  The  loga- 
rithms always  give  the  exact  intervals.  The 
decimal  point  is  omitted. 

6.  In  the  last  colunm  is  given  a  variety  of 
information.  The  name  of  the  interval  when 
any  usual  name  exists,  or  the  instrument  on 
which  it  is  found.  The  Greek  and  Arabic  in- 
tervals were  theoretical,  and  given  in  terms  of 
lengths  of  string.  As  we  see  from  sect.  B. 
No.  I,  p.  44 id,  there  is  every  reason  to  suppose 
that  the  real  intervals  tuned  differed  from 
them  materially.  Some  further  information  is 
occasionally  added. 

7.  If  the  interval  is  found  in  the  Duode- 
narium  (sect.  E.,  p.  463),  then  a  mode  of  obtain- 
ing it  by  Fifths  up  and  down  with  major 
Thirds  up  and  down  is  annexed.    Here 


Vu s one  Fifth  up  <=  702  cents,       2Vu^2x  702  cents,  &o. 

Yd  —  one  Fifth  down  «  498  cents  up,  2  7(i = 2  x  498  cents,  <feo. 

Tu  =  one  major  Third  up      =  386  cents,       2rii  =  2  x  386  cents,  <fto. 
Tt^Bone  major  Third  down  -=814  cents,       2Td »  2  x  814  oents,  Ac 


When  such  additions  are  made,  1200  or  mul- 
tiples of  1200  must  be  subtracted  till  the  result 
is  less  than   1200.    That  result  will  be  the 


number  of  cyclic  cents  in  the  first  column.  Of 
course,  if  we  take  the  value  to  three  places  of 
decimals. 


Ftt  =  7oi-95S.  Fd-498-045»  T«*-386-3i4,  r<f=8i3-686. 


and  then  the  result  will  be  correct  to  at  least 
two  places  of  decimals. 

8.  If  we  put  Ftt«?,  2Ftt=3*   ^0,  pa,  a, 

2  2«  3 

2Yd  =  ?,  Ac.  Tu  =  5,    2Tu  =  i*  Ac.   Td^^, 
3*  4  4»  5 

2Tda^,  Ac.  and  multiply  instead  of  adding, 

and  finally  multiply  or  divide  by  2  until  the 
c  result  lies  between  i  and  2,  these  formulsB  give 
the  exact  ratio.  Thus  2  7(2  •i'2Tu»  cyclically 
2  X  498  +  2  X  386  =  996  +  772  =  1768  - 1200  =  568 
cents  as  in  the  table.  Or  to  three  places  of 
decimals,  2x498-045 +  2x386-3 14 =996090  + 
772-628  =  1768718 -1200= 568-718,   which    is 

correct.    Or  again,  ^x  5!.iiL?|«?|  x  2  =  ?-5 
3«    4«    9  X  16    36  18 

the  correct  result.    See  Table  I.  under  568. 

9.  In  Table  11.  are  given  all  the  unevenly 
numbered  harmonics  up  to  the  63rd  in  order  of 
occurrence.  The  first  column  gives  the  number 
of  the  harmonic,  in  which  those  marked  *  will 
be  found  on  the  Harmonical  as  harmonics  of 
both  C  66  and  C  132,  and  those  marked  f 
as  harmonics  of  C  132  only.  In  the  second 
column  are  the  pitch  numbers  of  all  the  har- 
monics of  C  66.  In  the  third  column  Mog' 
are  the  logarithms  of  the  harmonics  of  I,  pre- 


ceded by  a  T^lvA  sign  + ,  so  that  if  to  each  of 
these  be  added  the  logarithm  of  the  pitch 
number  of  the  fundamental  note,  the  result  is 
the  logarithm  of  the  pitch  number  of  the  har- 
monic. Thus  Iqg  66=1-81954,  which  added 
to  1*36173,  the  log  opposite  23rd  harmonic, 
gives  3-i8i27»log  15 18,  the  pitch  number  (in 
the  table)  of  the  23rd  harmonic  of  C  66.  The 
column  is  divided  into  octaves  by  cross  lines, 
at  the  beginning  of  which,  preceded  by  a  minus 
sign  — ,  is  the  number  to  be  subtracted  from 
the  log  given  in  order  to  find  the  log  of  the 
harmonic  reduced  to  one  octave  as  given  in 
Table  I.  Thus  for  23rd  harmonic  1-36173— 
1*20412  =  '1 576 1,  which  is  the  log  opposite 
628  oents  in  Table  I.  In  the  fourth  column  is 
given  the  cyclic  cents  in  the  ratio  of  the  funda- 
mental note  to  the  harmonic  reduced  to  the 
same  octave,  the  same  as  given  in  Table  \^ 
where  will  be  found  the  more  exact  number 
of  cents.  But  to  each  octave  in  prefixed  the 
number  of  cents,  followed  by  a  j^lm  sign  + » 
which  have  to  be  added  in  order  to  find  the 
unreduced  interval.  Thus  for  23rd  harmonic  it 
is  4800  +  628  =  5428  cents.  Finally  in  the  last 
column  there  is  given  the  ntareni  equally  tem> 
pered  tone,  supposing  the  fundamental  note  is 
C,  and  the  number  of  cents  to  be  added  to  or 
subtracted  from  that  note  in  order  to  prodnoe 


Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


EOT.  D. 


MUSICAL  INTERVALS. 


453 


le  harmonic.  Thus  the  23rd  harmonic  is 
iiarper  than  \\f"%  by  28  cents.  These  com- 
arisonB  are  readily  made  from  the  column  of 
yolio  cents,  and  can  be  easily  applied  to  any 
uidamental  note.  Thas  the  23rd  harmonic 
f  B,b  must  be  4  Octayes  and  6  Semitones 
nd  28  cents  sharper  than  JB^b,  and  hence 
lust  be  e^"  +  28.  The  marking  of  the  differ- 
cLces  of  the  harmonics  from  equally  tempered 
otes  is  convenient  for  repeating  the  experi- 
lents  in  pect.  N.  No.  2. 

10.  Table  III.  is  constructed  to  shew  how 
f  ten  each  principal  interval,  not  exceeding  a 
^ritone,  is  contained  in  the  Octave.    The  first 


column  gives  the  cyclic  cents  in  the  interval 
for  easy  reference  to  Table  I.  The  second 
column  contains  the  names  of  the  intervals. 
The  third  contains,  up  to  one  place  of  deci- 
mals, the  number  of  times  that  the  interval  is 
contained  in  the  Octave,  found  by  dividing 
log  2  by  the  logarithm  of  the  interval  as  given 
in  Table  I.  This  is  therefore  not  always  the 
same  as  the  number  arrived  at  by  dividing 
1200  by  the  number  of  q/cUc  cents,  but  only 
by  the  number  of  precise  cents,  as  given  in 
Table  I.  Thus,  takmg  the  skhisma  of  2  cyclic 
and  1*953  ex.  cents,  1200+ 2 » 600,  too  small, 
but  i20o-ri'953  =  614-4  as  in  Table  III. 


I.  Table  of  Intervals  not  exceeding  one  Octave, 


Interral  Batios 
tAppTozimatiye 


Logs 


Name,  &o. 


I  :  I 
1730 :  1731 

32768 :  32805 

25s :  256 

95:96 

2025  :  2048 
80:81 

524288 :  531441 
63:64 

3072  :  3125 

48  -.49 

125  :  128 

39:40 

38:39 
37:38 
36:37 
35 :  36 
\     239 ;  246 

32:33 

31  :32 

30 :  31 

24:25 

\       67  :  70 

20 :  21 

19 :  20 

243 :  256 

128 :  135 


o 
00025 

00049 
00170 

00455 

00490 
00540 

00589 
00684 

00743 

00896 
01030 

01 100 

01 128 
01 1 58 
01 190 
01223 
01254 

01336 

01379 
01424 

01773 
01908 

021 19 

02228 

02263 

02312 


Fundamental  note  of  the  open  string,  assumed  as  C  66 
Cent,  hundredth  of  an  equal  Semitone,  nearest  approxi- 
mate ratio,  ex.  i  :  1*0005755 
Skhisma,  SVu+Tu=^C  :  B,5  ,  ex.  1*953 

Ex.  6775,  the  ratio  =i|  •  i|,  and  the  result  is  the  17th 
10    10 
harmonic  of  2>'b,  a  diatonic  Semitone  above  C 

Ex.  18*128,  the  ratio  is  -  •  — ,  or  the  interval  by  which 

the  19th  harmonic  is  flatter  than  the  minor  Third 
Diaskhisma,   4Vd  +  2Td  «  C  :  Dbb  =  C,J  :  D'b,    ex, 

19-553 
Comma  of  Didymus,  which  is  always  meant  by  Comma 

when  no  qualification  is  added,  ^Vu+Td^C  :  C\  ex. 

21-506 
Pythagorean    Comma,    12  7tt  »  C  :  Bff  «  D  b  :  C8  ,    ex. 

23*460 
Septimal  Comma,  or  interval  by  which  the  7th  harmonic, 

969  cents,  is  flatter  than  the  minor  Seventh,  996  cents, 

''Bt> :  Bb t  ex.  27264 
Small  Diesis,  Vd^  sTu^C-b  :  Bjl ,  ex.  29*614.    In  equal 

temperament  this  last  interval  would  be  represented  (as 

||JB  ;  c)  by  a  Semitone  of  100  cents 
Interval  of  Al  Farabi's  improved  Kabab 
Great  Diesis,  the  defect  of  3  major  Thirds  from  an  Octave, 

the  interval  between  CZ  and  Db  in  the  meantone  tem- 
perament, 3rd  =  Ca5  :  -D'b ,  ex.  41*059 
First  interval  on  the  Tambur  of  Bagdad,  the  interval  by 

which  the  13th  harmonic  of  840*528  cents  is  flatter  than 

the  just  major  Sixth  of  2^84*359,  ex.  43-831 
Second  interval  on  the  Tambur  of  Bagdad 
Third 
Fourth 

Fifth  „  „  „  „ 

Quartertone  of  Meshaqah,  the  quarter  of  an  equal  Tone, 

ex.  ratio -» I  :  1*0293022,  ||0  :  Cq 
33rd  harmonic,  interval  by  which  the  nth  harmonic  ex- 
ceeds the  just  Fourth,  F  :  *'^,  ex.  53*273 
Greek  Enharmonic  Quartertone,  supr4,  p.  265a 
Another  Greek  Enharmonic  Quartertone,  supri,  p.  265a 
Small  Semitone,  Vd  +  iTu^C  :  C^ff  ,  ex.  70*673 
Meantone  Small  Semitone,  meantone  C  :  CS  1  and  hence 

the  9  of  that  system,  ex.  76*050,  ex.  ratio  i  :  1*0449 
Snbminor  Second,  Greek  intervcd,  sapr^,  p.  264a ;  A^  :  'Bb, 

on  the  harmonical 
Interval  from  open  string  to  second  fret  on  the  Tambur  of 

Bagdad 
Pythagorean  Limma,  the  *  defect '  of  two  major  Tones,  408 

cents,  from  a  Fourth,  498  cents,  sVd  •  C  :  JDb,  ex. 

90*225 
Larger  Limma,  the  defect  of  a  Fourth,  498  cents,  increased 

by  a  diatonic  Semitone,  112  cents  (total  610  cents),  from 

a  Fifth,  702  cents,  and  hence  the  interval  by  which  the 


IT 


Digitized  by 


^.joogle 


454 


ADDITIONS  BY  THE   TRANSLATOR. 
Tablk  of  iKTEuviOiB  MOT  BZOBEDiNa  ONB  OcTAVB — conUntied, 


APP.  XX, 


C>o.io         InterviU  Hallos 
Cento        tApprozinuktiye 


IT 


94 
99 

100 


105 
112 

114 


117 

128 
134 


13s 

139 
145 
146 
150 


155 

165 

168 
180 
182 

193 


200 
204 
224 
231 

240 


246 
250 
251 
267 

274 

281 
i  294 


18 :  19 

17 :  18 
84:89 


16 :  17 
15 :  16 

2048 :  2187 


100 :  107 

13:  14 
25:27 


37:40 

12 :  13 

149 :  162 

4235 :  4608 

221  :  241 

II :  12 


32:35 
10 :  II 

49:54 

59049 :  65536 

9 :  10 

t  161 :  180 


400:449 
8:9 

225  :  256 
7:8 

74:85 

125  :  144 
200 :  231 

32 :  37 
108 :  125 

6:7 
64:7s 

17  :  20 
27  :  32 


02348 

02480 
02509 


02633 
02803 

02852 


02938 
03219 
03342 

03386 

03476 
03633 
03666 

03763 
03779 


03892 

04139 

04219 
04527 
04576 

04846 


05017 

0511S 
05606 
05799 

06021 


06145 
06271 

06305 
06349 
06695 

06888 

07158 
07379 


Name,  Ao. 


Fourth  must  be  sharpened  to  be  a  diatonic  Semitone 
below  (i.e.  the  *  leading  note '  to)  the  Fifth,  and  hence 
the  interval  by  which  the  Fourth  is  sharpened  on  moda- 
lating  into  the  dominant,  3VU+  Tu^  C  :  C,8  » ^  .  J^,| , 
ex.  92*179.  This  was  used  as  the  meaning  of  8  in  the 
first  English  edition,  for  which  in  the  present  114  cents, 
or  the  Apotome,  has  been  substituted,  to  agree  with 
Prof.  Helmboltz's  notation  for  just  intonation 

Greek  interval  in  Al  Farabi,  interval  between  the  i8th  and 
19th  harmonics,  d'"  :  ef"\>  on  Harmonical 

Arabic  interval 

Ex.  100-099,  ^e  nearest  approximate  in  small  numbers  to 
the  ratio  of  the  interval  of  an  equal  Semitone,  ex.  ratio 
I  :  1-059461 

17th  harmonic  »C  :  "d'"b  on  the  Harmonical,  ex.  104*955 

Diatonic  or  just  Semitone,  ex.  111731  cents,  Vd-i^Td=^ 
B  :  c^E  :  JF* 

Pythagorean  ApotomS,  'off-cut,'  or  what  is  left  of  the 
major  Tone,  204  cents,  after  *  cutting  off '  the  Limma  or 
90  cents,  used  for  t  in  this  edition,  7F«»C  :  Cf  «=C, 
:  C,«,ex.  113685 

Meantone  great  Semitone,  meantone  C  I  Z>b«  ex.  117-108 

Interval  between  13th  and  14th  harmonics,  ex.  128*298 

Great  Limma,  a  Ck)mma  greater  than  the  diatonic  Semi- 
tone, 112  cents,  ex.  133*237, 3Ft*+  2Td«C  :  D'b.  E^ :  F 
in  the  Phrygian  tetrachord,  supri,  p.  263<f\  No.  6 

Interval  from  open  string  to  the  thhni  fxet  of  the  Tambur 
of  Bagdad 

Interval  between  the  12th  and  13th  harmonics,  ex.  138*573 

Persian  *  near  the  forefinger '  lute  interval 

Arabic  *  near  the  forefinger '  lute  interval 

Meshaqah's  3  Quartertones,  imitation  of  151  cents,  ex. 
ratio  I  :  V2»=  I  :  1*0905 

The  interval  between  the  nth  and  12th  harmonics  on 
the  trumpet  scale,  used  in  Ptolemy's  equal  diatonic 
mode,  snpr&,  p.  2646,  used  by  Zalzal  in  Arabic  Inte  scale 
as  '  middle  finger,*  see  infri,  sect.  K.,  Persia,  Arabia,  and 
Syria 

The  35th  harmonic,  septimal  or  submajor  Second,  snpri, 
p.  2I2C,  ex.  155*140 

A  trumpet  interval,  used  in  Ptolemy's  equal  diatonic  scale, 
supra,  p.  2646 

Zahsal's  *  near  the  forefinger '  on  Arabic  lute 

Abdulqadir's  substitute  for  Zalzal's  168  cents 

The  minor  Tone  of  just  intonation,  the  *  grave  Seoond  *  of 
the  major  scale,  2F(2+  Tu«  C  :  /)i,  ex.  182-404 

The  mean  Tone,  the  Tone  of  the  meantone  system,  C  I  D, 
the  mean  between  a  major  Tone  of  204  cents,  and  a  minor 
Tone  of  182  cents,  ex.  ratio  i  :  ^^/s^^i  :  1*1180340 

An  equal  Tone,  ex.  ratio  i  :  '9'2s^  i  :  1*22462 

The  9th  harmonic,  major  Tone,  2FttaC  :  D,  ex.  203*910 

Diminished  minor  Third,  2Vd  +  2Td  =^B^  :  D* b, ex.  223-463 

Supersecond,  or  septimal  Second,  'Bb  :  c,  on  the  Har- 
monioal,  ex.  231*174 

The  Pentatone,  or  fifth  part  of  an  Octave,  ex.  ratio 
I  :  {/2«i  I  :  1*1487  in  the  Salendro  scale,  see  infri,  sect. 
E.,  Java 

Acute  diminished  minor  Third,  2F«  +  3T(2-B,  :  i>*b,  ex. 
244*968 

Five  Quartertones,  on  Mesh&qah's  scale,  ex.  ratio  i  :  '^2* 
«i  :  115535 

The  37th  harmonic,  ex.  251-344 

Grave  augmented  Tone,  3  V3  +  sTu  «  C  :  Djl ,  ex.  253*076 

Septimal  or  subminor  Third,  O  :  ^Bb  on  the  Harmonical, 
Poole's  minim  Third,  ex.  266*871 

The  75th  harmonic,  augmented  Tone,  Vu  t  iTw^  C  :  Djg , 
ex.  274*583 

Interval  on  Tambfir  of  Bagdad 

Pythagorean  minor  Third,  ancient  'middle  finger'  00 
Arabic  lute,  3Va»C  :  JSb,  ex.  294135 


Digitized  by 


\^oogle 


MUSICAL  INTEEVALS. 
Tabli  01  IimtBVAiiii  NOT  ixcBBDiNO  OMS  OoTATX — eontinutd. 


455 


Intenral  Batlos 
tApproximatire 


Logs 


Name,  &o. 


512 
520 
550  It 

551 
568 

583 


16 :  19 

37:44 

68:81 

5:6 

14: 17 

32 :  39 
59:73 


"S :  153 
22 :  27 

6561  :'8i92 
4:5 

50:63 
64:81 

25 :  32 

33 :  41 

7:9 

27:35 
10:  13 
96: 125 

16 :  21 

243  :  320 
25:33 


3:4 

227 :  303 

80: 107 

32:43 

20 :  27 

500 :  687 

8:  II 
18 :  25 

5:7 

32:45 


99: 140 

45:64 

512  :  729 

16 :  23 

25:36 

90:  131 
24:35 

32:47 
49:72 


177147 :  262144 

27:40 
109 :  163 

t  289 :  433 
2:3 


07463 
07526 

07598 
07918 
08432 

08591 
08648 


08780 

08894 

09642 
09691 

10034 
1023 1 

1072 1 
10763 
10914 

1 1288 

"394 
11464 

11810 

1 1954 

I204i 


12494 
12543 

12629 

12832 
13033 
13797 
13833 

14267 

I46I3 
14806 


15052 
15297 
15346 
1 5761 
15836 

16305 
16486 

16695 
I67I3 


17070 

17474 

17560 
17609 


The  19th  hannonio,  ex.  297*513 

The  equal  minor  Third,  pL  :  C 

Persian  *  middle  finger '  on  late 

JuBt  minor  Third,  Vu+Td^Ail  O'^O  :  E^b^ex,  315*641 

Wide  or  superminor  Third  in  the  chord  of  diminished 

Seventh,  'b"b  I  "d'"b  on  Harmonical,  ex.  336-129 
The  39th  harmonic,  ex.  342-483 
Arabic  late  open  string  to  string  of  the  mean  of  the 

lengths  for  204  and  498  cents,  practical  sabstitute  for 

355  cents 
Meshaqah's  7  Qaartertones,  tempered  form  of  355,  ex. 

ratio  I  :  ^^2'=  i  :  1*2241 
Zalzal's  *  middle  finger,*  or  wostd,  mean  length  of  strings 

for  303  and  408  cents 
Abdalqadir's  substitute  for  355  cents 
The  5th  harmonic,  just  major  Third,  Tu^C'.E^,  ex. 

386-314 
Equal  major  Third,  ex.  ratio  i  :  1^2«=  i  :  1-25992 10 
Pythagorean  major  Third,  or  Ditone,  as  it  consists  of  two 

major  Tonesa2  x  204,  ex.  407*820 
Diminished  Fourth,  iTd^E^  :  A^bt  ex.  427*342 
The  41st  harmonic,  ex.  429*062 
Septimal   or  supermajor  Third,   Poole's   maxim  Third, 

^Bb  :  don  Harmonical,  ex.  435*084 
Meahaqah's  9  Qaartertones,  ex.  ratio  'Vs'^  1*297 
One  of  Prof.  Preyer's  trial  intervals 
Superfluous    Third,   Vd  +  ^Tu^C  :  E^  ^^A^b  :  C,8  ,  ex. 

456-986 
The  2 1st  harmonic,  the  septimal  or  Bubfourth,  F  i^Bb  on 

Harmonical,  ex.  470*781 
Grave  Fourth,  $Vd  +  Tu=0  :  F^,  ex,  476*539 
Two  Pentatones,  the  representative  of  the  Fourth  in  the 

Salendro  scale,  see  infr4,  sect.  K.,  Java,  ex.  ratio  i  :  Ji^^ 

=  1:1*3195 
Just  and  Pythagorean  Fourth,  Vd^'^C  :  Fj  ex.  498*045 
Equal  Fourth,  ||C  :  J'',  ex.  ratio  i  :  »|/2»=  i  :  1*3348 
Meantone  Fourth,  meantone  C  :  F,  ex.  503-422,  ex.  ratio 

I  :  Jx  (^^;-A=«  :  1-3375 
The  43rd  harmonic,  ex.  511*5x8 
Acute  Fourth,  3Vu+Td  =  C  :  F^  =  A^  :  D,  ex.  519*552 
Meshaqah*8  ii  Quartertones,  ex.  ratio  I  :  'V2'*ai  :  1*374 
The  nth  harmonic,  ex.  551*318 
Superfluous  Fourth,  2Vd+2Tu^C  :  Fjl ,  ex.  568*718 
Septimal  or  subminor  Fifth,  E  :  ^Bb  on  Harmonical,  ex. 

582512 
The  45th  harmonic,  Tritone,  false,  sharp,  augmented,  or 

pluperfect    Fourth,    2  Ft*  +  Tt*  =  F ;  B,  =  C  ;  F,5  ,    ex. 

590*224,  the  Fourth  C  :  J^  as  widened  for  passing  into 

the  key  of  the  dominant,  498  +  92  »  590 
Equal  Tritone,  ^F  :  B,  ex.  ratio  i  :  V2^  i  :  1*4142 
Diminished  Pifth,2Fd+  !r(i  =  C  :  G*b  =-F>5  :  c,  ex.  609-777 
Pythagorean  Tritone,  6Vu=C  iFt^FiB,  ex.  611-731 
The  23rd  harmonic,  ex.  628*274 
Acute   diminished    Fifth,  2Vu  +  2Td»C  :  O^b  »ii|  :  e^b, 

ex.  631*283 
Meshaqah's  13  Quartertones,  ex.  ratio  i  :  'V2'*=i  :  1*4556 
Septimal,   or    Subfifth,  ^*b  :  ^-Bb    on    Harmonical,    ex. 

653-184 
The  47th  harmonic,  ex.  665*507 
Arabic  lute,  2nd  string,  a  Fourth  above  168  cents,  ex. 

666*258,  and  hence  *75i  cents  sharper  than  the  last,  the 

confusion  with  the  former  is  due  to  approximations 
Abdulqadir's  substitute  for  666  cents,  being  a  Fourth  above 

his  180  cents 
Grave  Fifth,  ^Vd+Tu^C  :  G„  ex.  680-449 
Meantone  Fifth,  meantone  C  :  G,  a  quarter  of  a  Comma 

too  flat,  ex.  696*579,  ex.  ratio  i  :  ]  x  4^{}- 1  :  1*4954 
Equal  Fifth,  |iC  :  O 
Just  and  Pythagorean  Fifth,  Vu=C:0,  ex.  701*955 


IT 


ir 


Digitized  by  VjOOQIC 


4S6 


ADDITIONS  BY  THE  TRANSLATOR. 
Tabls  or  iNTERVjOiB  NOT  BzciEDiNa  ONE  OcTkYE— conHfiiied. 


APP.  XX. 


Cyclic 
OentB 


Interval  Batios 
tApproximatiTe 


720 

724 
738 

772 

792 

794 
800 


t   95 :  144 

160 :  243 
32:49 
59:91 
16 :  25 

81 :  128 

256 :  40s 

63 :  100 


807 
814 
841 
850 

874 
882 


900 
906 
919 
926 

933 
938 
947 

950 

954 
960 

969 

976 
996 

999 
1000 

1018 

1030 
1050 
1059 
1067 
1088 

1 100 
1 1 10 
nil 
1117 
1 129 
1145 
1150 
1158 
"73 
1 180 
1200 


i! 

30: 

II : 

32: 

19683: 

3: 
22 : 

16: 

10 : 

75: 

7: 

32: 

125: 

93: 
72: 
85: 


:  13 

49 

18 
32768 

5 
37 

27 

17 

128 

12 

55 
;  216 

161 

148 


4:7 

128 :  225 
9 :  16 

32 :  57 
55:98 

5:9 

16  :  29 

6:  II 

32:  59 

27:50 

8:15 

89:  168 
128 :  243 
10 :  19 
32 :  61 
25:48 
16;  31 
35:68 

64: 125 

32:63 

1024 :  2025 

I  :  2 


Log! 


18062 
18149 

;K 

19382 

19873 
19922 
20069 


30242 
20412 
21085 
21323 

21388 
21913 
22136 

22185 
22577 

22724 

23045 

23215 

23408 
23521 
23754 

23831 
23958 
24082 


24304 

24497 
24988 

25072 
25086 

25527 

25828 
26340 
26570 
26761 
27300 

27594 
27840 

27875 
28018 
28330 
28724 
28848 

29073 
29419 
29613 
30103 


Kame^  Ac. 


Three  Pentatones,  giving  an  aoate  Fifth,  as  in  SalendiOi 

Bee  infri,  sect.  K.,  Java,  ex.  ratio  i  :  i^8  =  i  :  1*5157 
Acute  Fifth,  sVu+Td  =  C  :G\  ex.  723-461 
The  49th  harxnonio,  ex.  737*652 

Meshaqah's  15  Quartertones,  ex.  ratio  i  !  'V2**»  I  :  1*5424 
Grave  superfluoos  Fifth,  2Tu  =  C  :  (7,8  ,  the  25th  harmonic, 

ex.  772*627 
Pythagorean  minor  Sixth,  ^Vd^C  :  ^b,  ex.  792-180 
Extreme  sharp  Filth,  4Vu  +  Tu=C  :  O^t ,  ex.  794*134 
Equal  superfluous  Fifth,  ||C  :  (?S ,  and  also  equal  minor 

Sixth,  |)C  :  ^b,  the  same  notes  differently  written,  ex. 

ratio  I  :  ■^4^1  :  1*5874 
The  5 1  St  harmonic,  ex.  807*304 
Just  minor  Sixth,  Td»C  :  A^b,  ex.  813*687 
The  13th  harmonic,  ex.  840*528 
Meshaqah*s  17  Quartertones,  his  tempered  substitute  for 

853  cents,  ex.  ratio  i  :  ■^2"=i  :  1*6339 
Arabic  lute,  the  Fourth  above  Zalzal's  355  cents 
The  53rd  harmonic,  ex.  873*504 
Abdulqadir*B  substitute  for  Zalzal*8  853  cents,  being  a 

Fourth  above  384  cents 
Just  major  Sixth,  Vd  +  Tu^C  :  -4,,  ex.  884-359 
Equal  major  Sixth,  ||C  :  ^,  and  also  equal  diminished 

Seventh,  ||C  :  Bbb'^A  :  Gb, ex. ratio  i  :  ^8=1  :  1-6818 
The  27th harmonic,  Pythagorean  major  Sixth,  ^Vu^C  :A, 

ex.  905865 
Batio  of  the  loth  :  17th  harmonic,  the  harmonic  dimin- 
ished Seventh,  e/' :  "d"'  on  the  Harmonical,  ex.  918*641 
Just  diminished  Seventh,  Vd  +  2Td^C  :  B'bb  -^  :  G'b. 

ex.  925*416 
Septimal  or  supermajor  Sixth,  ^Bb  Iff,  ex.  933*129 
The  55ih  harmonic,  ex.  937*632 
Acute   diminished    Seventh,    ^'^u+S^^'^O  :  B*bb  ^A: 

0*bj  ex.  946*924 
Meshaqah's  19  Quartertones,  ex.  ratio  i  :  ^^2"si  :  1*7311 
Just  superfluous  Sixth,  sVd  +  ^Tu^C  :  Ajt ,  ex.  955*031 
Four  Pentatonea,  the  fourth  note  in  the  Salencbo  soUe, 

see  infri,  sect.  E.,  Java,  a  close  approximation  to  969 

cents,  ex.  ratio  i  :  •^16  ai  :  1*7411 
The  7th  harmonic,  natural,  harmonic,  or  subminor  Seventh, 

C  :  'Bb  on  the  Harmonical,  ex.  968*826 
Extreme  sharp  Sixth,  2F«  +  2Ttt»C  :  ^^  ,ex.  976-537 
Minor  Seventh,  used  in  the  subdominant,  2Vd^C  :  B^, 

ex.  996*091 
The  SJih  harmonic,  ex.  999*468 
Equal  superfluous,  or  extreme  sharp  Sixth,  i|C  :  ilt ,  or 

minor  Seventh,  ||0  :  Bb 
Acute  minor  Seventh,  used  in  descending  minor  scales, 

2Vtt+Td-C:B'b,ex.  1017*597 
The  29th  harmonic,  ex.  1029*577 

Meshaqah*s  21  Quartertones,  ex.  ratio  i  :  '^2'*  =  i  :  1*8340 
The  59th  harmonic,  ex.  1059-172 
Grave  major  Seventh,  ^Vd  +  2Tu^C  :  B„  ex.  1066-762 
Just  major  Seventh,  Ftt+  T^»C  :  B|,  the  15th  harmonio, 

ex.  1088*269 
Equal  major  Seventh 

Pythagorean  major  Seventh,  $Vu^C  :  B^ol.  1109*775 
One  of  Prof.  Preyer's  trial  intervals 
The  6iBt  harmonic,  ex.  1116*884 
Diminished  Octave,  Vu  +  2Td  »  C  I  C*b  *  ex.  1 129-327 
The  31st  harmonio,  ex.  1145*036 

Meshaqah's  23  Quartertones,  ex.  ratio  i  :  'V2*'»  i  :  1*943 
Superfluous  Seventh,  sTu^C  :  Bjt ,  ex.  11 58-941 
The  63rd  harmonio,  ex.  1172*736 
The  double  Tritone,  4F«  +  2Tii»  C  :  jB^  ,  ex.  1180*447 
The  Octave,  C  :  c 


Digitized  by 


Google 


.8X:CT.  E. 


MUSICAL  DUODENES. 


457 


Table  II.  The  Unevenly  Numbered  Harmonics  of  C  66  up  to  the  6^rd, 


No. 


•9 

*i5 


•19 

21 

23 

*25 

27 

•29 

31 


Pitch 


66 


198 


330 
462 


594 
726 
858 
990 


1 122 

1254 
1386 
1518 
i6|o 
1782 

1914 
2046 


Log 


-•30103 
+  •47712 


-•60206 
+  •69897 
+  •84510 


-•90306 
+  0-95424 
+ 1-04139 

+  111394 
+  1-17609 


-1-20412 

+  1-23045 
+  1-27875 
+ 1-32222 
+  1-36173 
+ 1-39794 
+  1-43136 
+ 1*46240 
+ 1-49136 


^^        Bqaal  notes 


1200  + 
702 


2400  + 
386 
969 


3600  + 
204 

841 
1088 


4800  + 

298 

471 

628 

772 

906 
1030 
1 145 


+  2 


tl       -14 

a'l    -31 


r 

a" 
h" 


+  4 
+  51 
-59 
-  12 


1 

e"i 

+  5 

d"'t 

-  2 

r 

-29 

/'"«+28    1 

f'l 

-27 

a"* 

+  6 

o'"«+30   1 

t/" 

+  45    1 

No. 

Pitch 

nos. 

33 

2178 

35 

2310 

37 

2442 

39 

2574 

41 

2706 

43 

2838 

45 

2970 

47 

3102 

49 

3234 

51 

3366 

53 

3498 

55 

3630 

57 

3762 

59 

3894 

61 

4026 

63 

4158 

Log 


Cyolio 


Equal  notes 


-1-50515 
+  1-5x851 

+  1-54407 
+  1-56820 
+  1-59106 
+  1-61278 

+  1-63347 
+  1-65321 
+  1-67210 

+  1-69020 

+  1-70757 
+  1-72428 
+  1-74036 

+  1*75587 
+  1-77085 

+  178533 
+ 1*79934 


6000  + 

53 

155 

251 

342 
429 
512 
590 
666 

738 
807 

874 

938 

999 
1059 
1117 
"73 


c~« 

-47 

d"' 

-45 

<rj 

-49 

dr-t 

+  42 

«'" 

+  29 

r 

+  12 

rt 

-10 

f 

-34 

^38 

9"l  +  7 
a'''  -26 
a'^  +38 
a"'«  -  I 
6^  -41 
b'"  +17 
c^     -27 


Table  HE.  Number  of  amy  Interval,  not  exceeding  the  Tritons,  contained  in  an 

Octave. 


Cyclic 

Number 

Cyclic 

Number 

cents  in 

Name  of  Interval 

in  an 

cents  in 

Name  of  Interval 

in  an 

interral 

Octare 

interval 

Octave 

2 

Skhisma 

614-3 

200 

Equal  Tone     . 

6-0 

20 

Diaskhisma    . 

61-4 

204 

Major  Tone    . 

5*9 

32 

Comma  .... 

55-8 

231 

Snpersecond   . 

5-2 

24 

Pyth.  Comma 

51-1 

240 

Pentatone       .        • 

S-o 

27 

Septimal  Comma    . 

440 

267 

Subminor  Third     . 

4*5 

28 

Small  Diesis  • 

40-5 

294 

Pyth.  minor  Third  . 

4*> 

42 

Great  Digsis  . 

29-2 

300 

Equal  minor  Third 

4-0 

50 

Qnartertone    . 

24-0 

316 

Just  minor  Third    . 

3-8 

70 

Small  Semitone 

17-0 

336 

Superminor  Third  . 

3-6 

76 

Meantone  small  Semitone 

15-8 

355 

Zalzal's  wostd 

3*4 

85 

Sabminor  Second    . 

14-2 

400 

Equal  major  Third 

4-0 

90 

Limma    .... 

13-3 

408 

Pyth.  major  Third  . 

2-9 

92 

13-0 

t^ 

Supermajor  Third  . 

2-8 

100 

Eqoal  Semitone 

I2-0 

Just  Fourth     . 

2-4 

112 

Just  Diatonic  Semitone  . 

IO-7 

500 

Equal  Fourth . 

2-4 

114 

Apotome 

10-6 

503 

Meantone  Fourth    . 

2-4 

117 

Meantone  great  Semitone 

IO-3 

590 

JustTritone    . 

2-0 

111 

Great  Limma 

9*o 

600 

Equal  Tritone 

2-0 

Minor  Tone    . 

6-6 

612 

Pyth.  Tritone 

3-0 

193 

Mean  Tone     . 

6-3 

SECTION  E. 

on  KUBICAL  DU0DBNS8  OB  THS  DBVELOPMEXIT  OW  JUST  INTONATION   FOB  HABUONT. 

(See  notes  pp.  3o8,  309,  211,  269,  272,  293,  298,  299,  301,  302,  304.  305,  306,  310,  333,  338.  345, 

346,  352,  and  363.) 

Art.      • 

1.  Introduction,  p.  458. 

2.  Harmonic  Elements,  p.  458. 

3.  Construction  of  the  Schemes,  p.  458. 

4.  Harmonic  Cell  or  Unit  of  Concord,  p.  458. 

5.  Harmonic  Heptad  or  Unit  of  Chord  Rela- 

tionship, p.  458. 


Art. 


6.  Harmonic  Decad  or  Unit    of  Harmony, 

7.  ChoMs  of  the  Decad,  p.  459. 

8.  Interyals  of  the  Decad,  p.  459. 

9.  Harmonic  Trichordals  and  Scales,  p.  460. 
10.  Principal  Trichordal  Scales,  p.  460. 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQ IC 


4S3  ADDITIONS  BY   THE   TEANSLATOR.  app.  xx. 

Art.  ^  Art. 

11.  Hannonlc  Daodene  or  Unit  of  Modulation,      i8.  The  Duodenarium,  p.  463. 

p.  461.  19.  Construction  of  the  Duodenarinm,  p.  463. 

12.  Modulation  into  the  Dominant  Duodene,      20.  Just  Intervals  reduced  to  steps  of  Fifths 

p.  461.  and  major  Thirds,  p.  464. 

13.  Modulation  into  the  Suhdominant  Duo-      21.  The  oolunm  of  Fifths,  p.  464. 

dene,  p.  462.  22.  The  Limits  of  the  Duodenarium,  p.  464. 

14.  Modulation  into  the    Mediant   Duodene,      23.  Introduction  of  the  Seventh  and  Seven- 

p.  462.  teenth  Harmonic,  p.  464* 

15.  Modulation  into  the  Minor   Submediant      24.  Need  of  Reduction  of  the  number  of  Just 

Duodene,  p.  462.  Tones,  p.  464. 

16.  Modulation  into  Relative  and  Ck>rrelative,      25.  Omission  of  the  Skhismas.     Unequally 

p.  462.  Just  Intonation.    The  oyde  of  53,  p.  465. 

17.  Duodenation,  p.  462.  26.  Duodenals,  p.  465. 

1.  Introduction.  The  following  sketch  is  founded  on  my  paper  on  the  same 
subject  in  the  Proceedings  of  the  Royal  Society  for  Nov.  19,  1874,  vol.  xxiii.  p.  3. 

^  It  is  an  attempt  to  develop  the  trichordal  principles  of  suprit,  pp.  293d  and  3093. 
This,  of  course,  is  an  inversion  of  what  actually  occurred.  But  the  introduction 
of  the  harmonic  principle  has  completely  changed  the  nature  of  music,  and  its  plaii 
consequently  requires  reconstruction.  Harmony  alone  is  considered.  Melody  is 
made  dependent  on  harmony.  The  harmony  is  tertian,  that  is,  it  includes  perfect 
Thirds,  major  and  minor ;  but  not  septimialy  that  is,  it  does  not  include  the  7th 
harmonic  of  the  base  of  a  chord.  But  this  may  be  superadded,  see  art  23.  The 
plan  here  pursued  also  has  the  advantage  of  showing  the  precise  tertian  relation  of 
the  notes  of  a  chord  written  in  the  usual  notation,  by  merely  superscribing  a  letter, 
called  the  duodenal,  without  any  change  whatever  in  the  ordinary  notation  itself. 
The  notes  affected  by  the  other  harmonies  can  then  be  easily  indicated  (art.  26) 

2.  The  Harmonic  Elements  are  the  intervals  of  a  Fifth,  major  and  minor  Third, 
with  all  their  extensions,  inversions,  and  extensions  of  their  inversions,  that  is  all 
the  forms  in  p.  191&,  c,  which  are  here  assumed.  Capital  letters  will  therefore 
indicate  notes  without  regard  to  octave,  and  even  allow  of  reduplication,  or  added 
octaves.     The  notation  is  otherwise  the  same  as  for  my  variation  of  Herr  A.  v. 

%  Oettingen's  notation,  explained  on  p.  277c,  note*,  and  used  through  the  rest  of 
this  work.  The  notation  of  intervals  is  used  as  in  p.  276^,  notet,  so  that  +  is 
386,  —  is  316,  ±  is  702,  I  is  294  cents,  and  (...)  is  replaced  by  the  proper 
number  of  cents  in  the  interval. 

3.  In  the  construction  of  the  schemes  notes  forming  ascending  Fifths  are 
written  over  one  another  vertically  ;  notes  forming  ascending  major  Thirds  are 
written  to  the  right  horizontally.  Against  each  note  is  written  the  number  of 
cycHc  cents  (supri,  p.  452a)  in  its  interval  from  G  or  the  root,  reduced  to  the  same 
octave.  A  notation  in  Solfeggio  terms  (modified  from  that  used  by  the  Tonic 
Solfedsts  with  Italian  pronunciation  of  the  vowels)  is  also  supplied,  in  which  Do 
stands  for  the  root  whatever  the  note  itself  may  be. 

4.  Harmonic  Cell  or  Unit  of  Concord. 

Letter  Notation,  Solfeggio  Notation. 

E^\}  316     G  702  Mo  316    So  70a 

%  C  o        El  386  Do  o        Mi  386 

This  consists  of  the  three  harmonic  elements,  the  Fifth  G±G  (or  Do±So)  and 
major  Thirds  C+E^ ,  ^*[>+G  (or  Do-^  Mi  and  Mo'\-So)  being  placed  as  already 
explained,  so  that  the  minor  Thirds  G—E^\}  (or  Do—Mo)  and E^^G  (or  1ft— So) 
lie  obliquely  upwards  to  the  left.  Such  a  cell  is  called  the  Cell  of  C  (or  Do)  its 
root. 

The  student  should  construct  such  cells  on  The  cell  contains  therefore  all  forms  of  the 

any  root.    C  has  been  adopted  simply  because  major  and  minor  chords,  triad  or  tetrad,  given 

it  is  most  usual,  and  because  it  is  suited  to  the  in  Chap.  XII.  above. 
Harmonical,  on  which  its  effect  should  be  tried. 

5.  The  Hoflrmonic  Heptad  or  Unit  of  Chord  BelationsMp. 

Letter  Notation.  Solfeggio  Notation. 

E^h  316     G  702  Mo  316    So  702 

A^\}  814    Co        Ex  386  Lo  814    Do  o        Mi  386 

i^  498    A^  884  Fa  498    La  884 

Digitized  by  VjOOQIC 


Solfeggio  Notation. 

To  1018    Re  204 

o88 

Mo    316    So  702     Ti  1088 

386 

Lo     814    Do  0        Jlfi   386 

884 

Fa  498    La   884 

E€T.  B.  MUSICAL  DUODENES.  45^ 

The  heptad  possesses  seven  notes,  whence  its  name.  It  is  formed  by  subjoining 
tie  cell  of  F  (or  Fa)  to  the  cell  of  G  (or  Do),  so  that  the  Fifth  of  the  lower  cell  is 
lie  root  of  the  upper  cell.  This  is  called  the  Heptad  of  C  (or  Do)  its  central  note. 
t  contains  not  only  the  4  cell  triads,  major  O+^i  — G.  -F+uli-C  (or  Do'\- 
^li—So,  Fa^La-Do),  minor  (7-E'[>+G^,  F  A^\}-^C  (or  Do-Mo'\'So,  Fa-* 
'^o-\-Do),  but  also  two  union  triads,  major  i4*t>+0— JS7*b  (or  Lo+Do— Jfo),  and 
ainor  A^^C-^Ei  (or  La— Do + Mi),  which  result  from  the  union  of  the  two  cells. 
t  therefore  possesses  all  the  six  consonant  chords  which  contain  the  same  note  G 
or  Do),  and  can  hence  pass  readily  into  one  another,  as  should  be  verified  on  the 
larmonical.  It  possesses  also  the  seven  condissonant  triads  (p.  338,  note  f)  con- 
aining  G,  the  major  Trine  i4*[>+CH-jB?,  (or  Lo+Do-^Mi)  containing  two  major 
[thirds,  and  the  ndnor  Trine  Ai  —  G^EJi  (pxLa—Do-^Mo)  containing  two  minor 
["hirds,  the  jpure  quintal  Trine  F±:G±G  (or  Fa±Do±So)  containing  two  Fifths, 
he  major  quintal  Trines  u4»p+0±G  (or  Lo-^Do±So)  and  F±G+E^  (or  Fa± 
Do-^Mi)  consisting  of  a  major  Third  and  a  Fifth,  and  the  minor  quintal  Trines 
li  — 0±G  (or  La—Do±So)  and  i''±C'— J^*t>  (or  Fa±Do—Mo)  consisting  of  a  ^ 
oinor  Third  and  a  Fifth. 

All  of  these  should  be  stadied  on  the  Har-      consonant  triad  containing  the  same  note  G 
lonioal,  and  the  readiness  with  which  their      should  be  felt. 
issonanoe  may  be  removed  by  passing  into  a 

6.  The  Harmonic  Decad  or  Unit  of  Hamu>ny. 

Letter  Notation, 
B»|>  1018    D  204 
E^\}    316     a  702    5, 
il»b    814    Go        E^ 

F    498         Ay 

The  Decad  possesses  ten  notes,  whence  its  name.  It  consists  of  the  heptad 
»f  G  (or  So)  superimposed  on  the  heptad  of  G  (or  Do).  These  two  heptads  have 
\  common  cell,  that  of  G  (or  Do).  Hence  the  decad  of  G  (or  Do)  may  also  be  ^ 
considered  as  three  cells,  the  tonic  or  that  of  G  (or  Do)  in  the  middle  ;  the  domi- 
lant  or  that  of  G  [ox  So)  above,  and  the  subdominant  or  that  of  i^  (or  Fa)  below. 
Che  decad  of  G  (or  Do)  is  the  complete  development  of  the  cell  of  G  (or  Do),  for 
he  root  of  the  upper  cell  is  the  Fifth  of  the  root  of  the  middle  cell,  while  that 
*oot  itself  is  the  fmh  of  the  root  of  the  lower  cell. 

7.  The  Ghords  of  the  Decad.  The  vertical  axis  is  the  column  of  Fifths  F±G±. 
T:tD  (or  Fa±Do^So±Be).  Those  are  two  horizontal  axes  of  major  Trines. 
Phe  decad  contains  3  cell  ?na;or  triads  F-^A^^G,  G-^Ei  —  G,  G-^B^^D  (or 
?a  +  La— Do,  Do  +  3ft— So,  §0+  Ti—Be)  on  the  right,  and  2  union  major  triads 
i>b+C7--B*b»  ^'b+^-^*b  (or  Lo-\'Do-Mo,  Mo-¥So-To)  on  the  left.  The 
lecadalso  contains  3  cell  minor  triads  F-A^^^+G,  G-E^\}'tG,  G-5^b  +  Z)  (or 
ya-Lo  -^^  Do,  Do— Mo  +  So,  So  —  To+Be)  on  the  left,  and  2  union  minor  triads 
ii-C+-&,,  Ei-G-^Bi  (or  La-Do+Mi,  Mi-So^Ti)  on  the  right.  It  has 
Jso  the  dissonance  of  the  dominant  Seventh,  G-^ B  —  D  \  F  (or  So  +  Ti  — 
He  I  Fa)  and  mimyr  Ninth  G+B-D  \  F—A^\}  (or  So-k-Ti-Be  \Fa-Lo),  andf 
lence  of  the  diminished  Seventh  (the  same  less  G  or  So),  and  of  the  added  Sixth 
f^+u4-C  204  D,  or  2^+4  520  D  {Fa+La-Do  204  Be,  or  Fa+La  520  Be),  but 
lot  the  minor  triad  2>,—  JF*+u4,  (or  Ba—Fa-k-La,  see  Ba  in  art.  11,  p.  461), 
^hich  is  confused  with  it  in  tempered  intonation.    And  it  has  also  not  the  chords 

>f  the  extreme diarp  Sixth,  D^\}'\'F  204  (t+5,,  p.  2866,  or/*  +a  590  d,J, p.  308&. 

8.  The  Intervals  of  the  Decad.  The  relative  position  of  the  principal  intervals 
ihould  be  observed  in  addition  to  the  vertical  Fifths  (including  Fourths),  the  hori- 
:ontal  migor  Thirds  (including  minor  Sixths),  and  oblique  minor  Thirds  (including 
najor  Sixths),  on  which  the  scheme  is  founded. 

Major  Tone  204,  two  Filths  vertically  np,  Diatonic  Semitone  112,  obliquely  down  to 

aC  D  (or  Do  Re).  the  left  in  the  next  line  as  B,  C  (or  Ti  Do). 

Minor  Tone  182,  obliquely  down  to  the  right  Small  Semitone  70,  obliquely  down  to  the 

D  the  next  line  but  one  as  G  il|  (or  So  La).  right  in  the  next  column  but  one,  and  in  the 

Defective  Fifth  680,  obliquely  down  to  the  next  line,  as  i^'b  B,  (or  To  Ti^. 
ight  in  the  next  line  but  two  as  D  Ai  (or  This  is  the  smallest  interval  occurring  in  a 

3e  La).  Decad. 

Digitized  by  V^OOQ IC 


46a  ADDITIONS  BY   THE   TRANSLATOR.  app.  xx. 

9.  Harmonic  Trichordals,  A  trichordal  consists  of  one  triad  from  each  of  the 
three  cells  of  a  decad.  Eight  such  trichordals  may  be  formed  from  the  three 
major  and  three  minor  cell  triads.     Abbreviate  the  names  '  major  triad '  and '  minor 

^triad '  into  their  first  syllables,  ma,,  mu  (wliich  however  may,  if  preferred,  be  read 
more  at  fall  as  '  major '  and '  minor  '),  and  name  them  in  the  order  of  Subdominant, 
Tonic  and  Dominant  triads,  then  the  eight  trichordals  are  ma^nia.ma.,  mi.ma.ma., 
via,mi.7na,,  mi,mi,ma,,  ma.ma.mi,,,  mi,ma,mi,,  ma,rm.mi,,  mi,ini,im.  The  seven 
tones  in  each  trichordal  reduced  to  a  single  octave  constitute  an  harmonic  scale,  thai 
is  a  scale  in  which  each  note  belongs  to  a  triad  in  the  scale.  We  may  begin  the 
scale  with  any  one  of  the  notes  of  any  one  of  the  3  generating  triads.  These  notes 
may  be  numbered  in  order  of  sharpness  when  reduced  to  the  same  octave,  as  4,  6,  i 
in  the  subdominant,  1,3,  5  in  the  tonic,  and  5,  7,  2  in  the  dominant.  These 
numbers  may  be  simply  prefixed  to  the  trichordal  they  affect,  to  shew  on  what 
note  the  scale  begins.  We  thus  obtain  7  x  8=56  trichordal  scales,  of  which  8  are 
%  generically  different,  each  genus  having  7  species.  In  some  accounts  of  the  modes 
they  are  all  represented  in  faxst  as  ma.ma.ma.,  differing  only  as  to  the  note  of  the 
scale  with  which  they  begin.  This  is,  of  course,  thoroughly  erroneous.  The  student 
is  recommended  to  make  out  on  the  Harmonical  every  one  of  these  56  scales. 

10.  Principal  Trichordal  Scales,  The  following  gives  a  list  of  the  principal 
scales  thus  generated  referring  to  the  places  where  they  have  been  noted  in  the 
text,  and  the  scale  is  noted  as  beginning  with  C, 

The  figures  between  two  oonseoutive  notes  the  Harmonical  a  form  is  given  which  can  be 

indicate  the  interval  between  them  in  cents,  played.     As  each  note  forms  part  of  a  cell 

The  number  prefixed  to  the  root  of  the  decad  triad,  and  mostly  also  of  a  union  triad,  each 

indicates  the  note  of  the  chord  with  which  the  scale  can  be  harmonised,  and  the    stadent 

scale  begins,  reckoned  in  the  way  just  men-  should  therefore  harmonise  all  of  them  on  the 

tioned.    Beferenoes  to  the  text  fpllow.    Where  Harmonical.    See  also  p.  277,  note  f.    No  ex- 

any  one  of  these  scales  cannot  be  played  on  amples  of  the  unusual  VI.  Mi.ica.mi.  are  given. 

I, 


m       I  0-C  204  D  182  J^i  112  J^  204  O  182  A^  204  B|  112  c,  No.  I  of  p.  2746  and  note.     Bfajor 
Mode  of  p.  2986.    Ordinary  C  major. 

K  F^C  182  D|  204  JE7|  112  F  204  G  182  A^  112  £b  204  c,  No.  5  of  p.  275a,  there  called  the 
mode  of  the  Fourth.  This  must  be  played  on  the  Harmonical  as  5  C  ma.majaia.,  which  has  the 
same  intervals,  namely:  O  182  Ai  204  B^  112  e  204  d  182  e^  112  /  204  y.  It  is  No.  5  ol 
p.  275a,  there  called  the  mode  of  the  Fourth. 

II.  MZ.1IA.MA. 

I  CsC  204  D  182  E^  112  F  204  G  112  A^b  274  B  112  c.  The  minor-major  mode  of 
pp.  3056  and  309^. 

III.  Ma.mi.ma. 

I  C  =  C  204  D  112  J^'b  182  ^  204  G  182  ill  204  B,  112  c,  No.  2  of  p.  2746.  The  mode  of  th« 
minor  Seventh,  with  the  leading  note,  or  major  Seventh,  substituted  for  ihe  minor  Seventh,  aa 
in  p.  303d.    An  ordinary  form  of  the  ascending  minor  scale  p.  288a. 

lY.  M1.MI.11A. 

I  C^C  204  D  ii2£7*b  182  JP  204(7  112  A^b  274  B,  112  c.  The  *  instrumental '  minor  seal* 
of  p.  2886 ;  the  *  modem '  ascending  minor  scale  of  p.  ^ood, 

IF  V.  Ma.ma.mi. 

I  C-C  204  D  182  £>,  112  F  204  G  182  Ai  134  B^b  182  c.  Although  this  is  called  the  mode 
of  the  Fourth  on  pp.  2986  and  309^2,  it  is  a  different  scale  from  that  called  the  mode  of  the 
Fourth  on  p.  2756,  which  is  5  J^  ma.ma.ma.  above,  under  I.,  because  the  Seventh  in  this  case  is 
JB'b  and  in  that  Bbt  a  comma  lower.    See  p.  277,  footnote  t,  on  this  and  similar  confusions. 

5  ^a  C  182  Di  134  ^^ b  182  F204G  iSzA^iii  Bb  204  c.  This  must  be  played  on  the  Har- 
monical as  5  C-G  182  ^,  134  B^b  182  c  204  d  182  e,  112  /204  pr.  This  is  No.  6  of  p.  275^ 
and  there  considered  as  a  variant  of  the  mode  of  the  minor  Seventh,  which  is  really  the  diffeieni 
scale,  3  C  majnioni.,  next  immediately  following. 

VII.  Ma.mi.mi. 

I  C«C  204  D  112  E^b  182  ^  204  G  182  ill  134  -S^b  182  c.  This  is  No.  4  of  p.  2750  taken 
upwards,  instead  of  downwards  as  there.  The  mode  of  the  minor  Seventh  of  p.  2986  withoni 
the  leading  tone  of  p.  303c. 

VIU.  M1.MI.MI. 

I  C'^C  204  D  112  E^b  182  F  204  G  112  il'b  204  B^b  182  c.  No.  3  or  descending  minor 
scale  of  p.  274c,  the  mode  of  the  minor  Third  of  p.  294a,  No.  4. 

5  F» C  112  jD'b  204  £*b  182  F  204  G  112  A^b  182  Bb  204  c.    This  must  be  played  on  tha 


Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


SECT.  E.. 


MUSICAL  DUODENES. 


Harmonica!  as  5  C^G  112  A^b  204  B*b  182  C  204  D  112  E^b  182  F  204  {7.    It  is  No. 
p.  2756,  the  mode  of  the  minor  Sixth  of  pp.  294a  No.  5,  298c  No.  5,  305c  to  308^. 


461 
7  of 


II.  The  Harmonic  Dtwdene,  or  Unit  of  Modulation. 

Letter  Notation,  Solfeggio  Notation. 


2Pb 

134 

F^      520 

^     906 

0.« 

92 

^^1478 

0«b 

632 
1130 

B^b  1018 
E'b   316 

i)     204 

F,% 

590 

^^976 

e«b 

G     702 

Bi 

1088 

I>JB^274 

ip^b 

428 

A'b    814 

C         0 

E, 

386 

OJI772 

.B*bb 

926 

D»b    112 

F    498 

^. 

884 

c,t  .  70 

^«bb 

224 

G'b    610 

Bb  996 

^1 

182 

F4568 

rtt 

So 

U 

\di 

me 

8U 

to 
mo 

re 

\fl 

li 

du 

\    80 

\U 

ri 

fu 

lo 

1   do 

I  mi 

«e 

tu 

ro 

1  fa 

la 

de 

mu 

1- 

\   ^ 

ra 

fe 

In  referring  to  the  figure  of  the  decad  in  art.  6  two  gaps  will  be  noticed,  one  to 
the  right  at  the  top,  the  other  to  the  left  at  the  bottom.  On  filling  these  up  accord- 
ing to  the  same  laws  by  making  F^jf^  a  Fifth  above  ^i  and  a  major  Third  above 
D,  and  D'|>  a  Fifth  below  A^\}  and  a  major  Third  below  F,  we  obtain  the  scheme 
in  the  central  rectangle  of  the  above  figures,  which  is  called  a  Dtiodene,  because  it 
consists  of  ttvelve  notes  bearing  to  each  other  the  relation  of  the  twelve  notes  on 
the  piano,  which,  by  omitting  the  marks  of  commas  ^1,  may  be  supposed  to  be 
represented  by  the  same  letters.  The  Duodene  then  consists  of  3  columns  or 
quaternions  of  Fifths,  and  four  lines  or  major  trines  of  Thirds,  and  its  root,  which 
is  tiiat  of  the  corresponding  decad,  is  in  the  centre  of  the  lowest  line  but  one,  so 
that  it  is  easy  to  construct  a  duodene  on  any  note  as  a  root. 

The  duodene  thus  completed  by  these  extreme  tones  possesses  two  additional 
union  Thirds,  major  D^\}'\'F—A^\}  (or  Bo-^-Fa^Lo)  and  minor  5,— D+i^iJf  (or 
Tt—Be-^Fi),  and  consequently  besides  the  8  genera  of  scales  of  the  decad  contains  T 
the  major  scale  of  il*|>  (major  chords  D»t?+^-^*bM*b+<^--25;»b.  E^h-^-G-B^l}) 
and  the  descending  minor  scale  of  Ei  (minor  chords  ul|  ^C-^-E,  E^  —  (r+^n  -Bi  — 
D-\-Filf)j  and  it  also  gives  us  the  chords  of  the  extreme  sharp  Sixth  (976  cents,  a 
yexj  near  approach  to  the  7th  harmonic  of  969  cents)  in  its  three  forms, 

Italian    Sixth  A^\}  386  C  590  i^i^  scarcely  a  dissonance, 
French  Sixth  A^\}  386  C  204  D     386  FA, 
German  Sixth  A^\}  386  C  316  E^\}  274  F^%. 

These  three  last  chords  cannot  be  played  on  the  Harmonical.  They  arose  in  the  days  of 
xneantone  temi)eTament.  The  chord  of  the  dominant  Seventh  omitting  the  Fifth  E^b  386 
O  610  ^'b  had  then  to  be  played  with  tempered  notes  as  Eb  386  O  579  cff ,  because  there  was  no 
db  on  the  instrument,  and  as  the  7th  harmonic  would  have  been  E^b  386  Q  583  'd*b  the  effect 
was  so  good  that  the  chord  was  adopted  in  writing  and  distinguished  from  the  chord  of  the 
dominant  Seventh,  by  resolving  upwards  instead  of  downwards. 

These  new  notes  have  also  introduced  two  new  Semitones  of  92  cents,  i^  92  F,  JJ  1[ 
and  D^\}  gi  D.    But  the  smallest  interval  between  any  two  notes  remains  the 
small  Semitone  of  70  cents,  A^\}  70  -4 1,  E^\}  70  E,,  B^[}  70  5,, 

12.  Modulation  into  the  Dominant  Duodene. 


It  is  obvious  from  the  construction  of  the 
duodene  that  the  transition  from  any  duo- 
dene to  an  adjacent  one  is  very  easy.  Suppose 
(see  scheme  in  art.  11)  that  we  omit  the  lowest 
line  D*b  +  F  +  ^,  (or  Ro-^Fa-^La)  and  take 
in  the  line  i^' +  ^  •«- C,S  at  the  top,  we  shall 
have  the  duodene  of  G,  which  has  three  lines 
in  common  with  that  of  O.  The  three  new 
notes  introduce  2 commas  F  22  F\  Ai22A  and 
one  diaskhisma  0,8  20  D'b.  These  minute 
distinctions  neglected  in  tempered  music  have, 
however,  a  powerful  effect  on  the  harmony  of 
justly  intoned  instruments.    The  C|S  is  indeed 


one  of  the  extra  notes  and  does  not  occur  in 
the  decad  of  O  or  its  scales.  On  the  otiier 
hand  F,S  ,  which  was  an  extra  note  in  the  duo- 
dene of  Cy  becomes  a  substantive  note  in  the 
decad,  as  well  as  duodene,  of  G,  and  we  find 
then  that  the  F  of  the  C  decad  becomes  the 
Fit ,  92  cents  higher  in  the  G  decad.  This 
difference  is  so  large  that  it  cannot  be  disre- 
garded in  tempered  music,  and  it  is,  accord- 
ingly, there  represented  by  an  interval  of  100 
cents,  and  forms  the  distinguishing  mark  for 
major  scales  of  what  is  termed  the  modulatum 
into  the  dominant  as  just  explained. 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


462 


ADDITIONS  BY  THE  TRANSLATOR. 


APP.  XX. 


13.  Modulation  into  the  Subdominant  Duodene. 


Omit  the  top  line  B^b  +  D  +  F^t  (or  To 
+  Re  +  Fi)  of  the  daodene  of  O  in  scheme 
art.  II,  and  take  a  new  line  G'b  +  Bb  + A  (o^ 
Sa  +  Ta  +  Ra)  at  the  bottom  to  obtain  the 
duodene  of  F  (or  Fa),  The  changes  made  are 
the  reverse  of  those  for  modulations  into  the 
dominant.  The  notes  B^  b,  D  are  depressed  by 
a  comma  of  22  cents  to  JBb»  i>i,  and  the  extra 
note  F^%  of  the  C  daodene  is  raised  by  a 


diaskhisma  of  20  cents  to  O^b  of  the  ^daodene. 
These  changes  are  neglected  in  tempered  in- 
tonation. Bat  the  most  important  change 
is  that  B^  (which  is  in  the  daodene,  bat  not 
the  decad  of  F)  is  depressed  by  a  Semitone  of 
92  cents  from  B,  to  Bb»  and  this  being  noticed 
in  tempered  masic,  becomes  the  distingoish- 
ing  mark  in  the  modulation  of  the  major  aeale 
of  C  into  that  of  the  subdominant  F, 


14.  Modulation  into  the  Mediant  Duodene, 


Betuming  again  to  the  daodene  of  C,  we 
might  omit  tibe  left  column  D^b±A^b±E^b± 
B*  b  (or  ro  ±lo  ±mo  ±to)  of  the  duodene  of  C 
[  (see  scheme  in  art.  ii)  and  introduce  a  new 
column  on  the  right  CjS  db  Q^  ±  D^  ± 
A^  (or  de±8e±ri±t%)y  in  which  each  new 
note  is  a  great  diesis  of  42  cents  lower  than 
the  note  in  the  column  omitted.    This  differ- 


ence is  ignored  in  playing  tempered  mnsie, 
although  the  distinction  is  preseired  in  writ- 
ing as  Db  and  C%  ,  <Jkc.,  bat  it  is  of  great  im- 
portance in  just  intonation.  This  is  termed 
modulation  from  the  root  C  (or  Ikf^  into  the 
Mediant^  E^  (or  Mi)  as  the  root  of  the  new 
daodene  is  the  major  Third  or  Mediant  of  the 
root  C  (or  Do)  of  the  old  daodene. 


15,  Modulation  into  the  Minor  Svhmediant  Duodene, 


Similarly  we  might  omit  the  right  column 
A^±E^±By±F^%  (or  la  mi  ti  fi)  of  the  duo- 
dene of  C  (see  scheme  in  art.   11)  and  in- 


troduce a  new  column  B-'bb  ±  F"b  ±C-b±  G*b 
^or  ta  fu  du  su)  on  the  left,  thus  forming  the 
duodene  of  A^  b  (or  Lo)  or  minor  subtnedianL 


16.  Modulation  into  the  Belative  and-  Correlative  Duodenes. 


But  it  is  usual  to  combine  these  two  modu- 
lations with  others  into  the  subdominant  of 
the  mediant  (that  is  the  submediant)  A^  (or 
La),  generally  called  the  relative  on  the  one 
m  hand,  and  the  dominant  of  the  minor  sub- 
mediant,  that  is  E^b  (or  Mo)y  sometimes  called 
the  correlative^  on  the  other.  In  each  case  the 
root  of  the  new  duodene  differs  by  a  minor 
Third  from  the  old  root.  The  change  for  these 
two  last  modulations  is  considerable  if  we  take 
the  whole  duodene  into  consideration,  as  may 
be  seen  by  the  schemes  in  art.  11,  where  the 
dotted  lines  mark  off  the  new  duodenes.  But  in 
the  cases  which  occur  in  practice  the  changes 
are  very  small,  especially  when  the  difference 
of  a  comma  is  neglected  as  in  tempered  music. 
The  modulation  into  the  relative  is  generaUy 
from  the  major  scale  of  the  decad  of  C  or  chords 
F+Ai-CC  +  Ei-O.O  +  B^-DinioHieminor 
scale  of  the  relative  decad  of  il„  either  consist- 
ing of  the  chords  D,  -  F+  i4„  -4,  -  C  +  ^,,-E?,  - 
O  +  Bi  (in  which  there  is  only  the  use  of  D,  for 
D,  to  indicate  this  modulation),  or  else  con- 
In  sistingof  the  chords  jD,-F+-4„  -4,-C  +  -B„ 
VE?,4Gjr-B„  or  even  D^  +  FJt-At,  A^- 
C-^Et  !&,  — Gjf— B„  these  being  the  three 
recognised  forms  of  the  minor  scale.  As  the 
two  latter  forms  are  also  acknowledged  in 


tempered  intonation  (which,  however,  confuses 
F,5  and  F.J  ),  the  change  of  O  into  G^  (or  so 
into  se,  that  is  the  sharpening  of  the  Fifth  by 
properly  70  cents)  has  generally  been  con- 
sidered the  mark  of  modulation  from  a  major 
scale  into  its  relative  minor,— one  of  the  com- 
monest in  music. 

The  modulation  into  the  correlative  is  in 
the  same  way  generally  thought  of  as  the 
change  of  the  tonic  major  scale  chords  F+ 
^,-C.,  C-*-E^-0,  G  +  Bi-D  (or  Fai^La 
-Do,  Do  +  Mi-So,  So+Ti-Re)  into  the 
tonic  minor  scale  chords  F—A^b+C,  C- 
E^b  +  G,G-B^b  +  D  (or  Fa-Lo^Do,  Do- 
Mo -¥  So,  So  — To  +  Re),  in  which  however  the 
chord  G  +  B^-D  (or  80+Ti-Re)  may  re- 
main, and  sometimes  the  chord  F+A^  —  O 
(or  Fa  La  Do),  This  is  not  a  modulation  at 
all  in  the  sense  just  explained,  because  there 
is  no  change  of  duodene  or  even  decad.  It  is 
merely  a  change  of  scale  within  the  same 
decad,  that  is,  a  new  triohordal  scale  moving 
from  I C  ma.ma.ma.  to  I  Cmijni  jni.,  mijnLma.. 
or  ma.mi.ma.,  art.  9.  It  would,  however,  be 
more  consonant  to  ancient  practice  to  restrict 
the  term  modulation  to  such  changes  of  tri- 
chordal  within  the  same  duodene,  and  to  use 
a  new  term  for  the  more  general  operation. 


17.  Dux)denation, 


This  is  the  term  I  propose  to  substitute 
for  modulation  when  it  means  passing  from 
one  duodene  to  another  which  bears  a  known 
relation  to  the  first.  This  relation  may  be  very 
close,  as  in  the  cases  just  considered,  or  so 
remote  that  the  two  duodenes  have  only  one 
note  in  common.  Thus  the  duodene  of  D'b 
and  C  have  only  the  note  B'b  in  common. 
The  annexed  figure,  called  the  Duod^ndi^um, 
probably  contains  all  such  duodenations  which 


occur  even  in  modem  music,  though  it  is  im- 
possible to  be  certain  how  far  the  ambiguities 
of  tempered  intonation  may  mislead  the  com- 
poser to  consider  as  related,  chords  and  scales 
which  are  really  very  far  apart.  It  contains 
therefore  an  approximative  estimate  of  117, 
for  the  number  of  tones  in  aa  Octave  which 
would  be  required  to  play  in  jast  intonation, 
and  are  roughly  represented  by  the  12  tones  of 
equal  temperament. 


Digitized  by  VjOOQIC 


BCT.  B. 


MUSICAL  DUODENES. 


463 


18.  The  Diiodendrium. 

The  large  figures  give  the  cychc  cents  in  the  interval  of  each  note  from  C.     See  Table 
440.    The  small  figures  give  the  corresponding  nunberB  of  the  cycle  of  53  with  the  nearest 
bole  number  of  cents.    See  Table  p.  439. 


I 

2 

3 

i 

* 

S 

6 

7^ 

8 

9 

-1-26 

"•94 

-63 

— 

•31 

0 

+  •31 

+  •63 

+  •94 

+  1-26 

B*bb 

970 

D*b      156 

JP«     542 

A' 

928 

c« 

114 

JS7Jsoo:G,t«886 

B4S 

72D,«««458| 

E^bb 

^ 

zz          158 

a»b    654 

aS       5431    45 

B*b  IP40,Z)» 

9a8 
226 

Fi 

612 

96   498)   43      883 
^»998Cjm84 

„7           681     34            453I 

^4«  570  G4««  95^1 

16 
A*bb 

766 

33     657 

so     104a     X4 

aa6 

3X 

6x1 

48    996,    xa       181 

89 

566 

40         95 1 

Cb    1152 

E^b   338 

G» 

724 

B 

1 1 10 

A«296 

F4«682il4«io68 

C4«««254 

38 
2>*bb 

770 
64 

a         IIS5 

F*b      450 

19       340 

4»b    836 

36 

7*5 
22 

53= 

so  XX09 

408 

17    894 

34     679 

5x 

X064 

X5           349 

F4««752 

G,t  794 

Bjl  ii8oD,«t  366 

G^bb 

<» 
562 

B^bb   94^ 

D»b    134 

F.' 

"3 

520 

aa 

A 

9^ 

^39     79a 
C|«     92 

3       11771    ao 

E^    478Gf,tt 

8^6^ 

37         749 
B4«       50 

«9 

C^bb 

566 

io6n 

46      951 

10     136 

«7 

5a« 

44 

906 

8      91 

85       475.    4a 

4JJ    976C,«J 

86a 
162 

6              45 

E,nt   548 

£;»bb   246 

a*b  632 

B«b 

IOI8Z) 

204 

F,«  590 

F?bb 

1064 
3S2 

15          a49 

4«bb    744 

C*b  I 130 

^t'b 

r.'^'a' 

904 

702 

bTi<^ 

47     974'    " 
D^    274F««jr 

6)5^ 

a8            543 

4,««   1046 

B*bbb 

i^ 

I^bb     42 

X      xi3a 

F'b   428 

z8 
4'b 

^^c" 

70a 
0 

E^  3t^ 

_i6       a7a     33 

QS    772  B,5 

w'^ 

50          104a 

D,nt   344 

E*bbb 

860 
i';4 

6            45 

a»bb  540 

as       430 

B'bb926 

Dt^b 

8x5    4 
112F 

498 

/:  8l1 

38       770      a 

c^     70  ^,« 

XI58 
456 

o]tt   ^° 

IK                 X58 

A^bbb  652 

a8         543 

C"bb  1038 

45       9*8 

£;«bb224 

9 

XX3,    a6 

49B 

43   883 

7         68 

F^    568 

a4 
4.» 

453 

954 

41      838 

C4«     140 

a»b 

610 

Bb 

996 

D,    182 

33 

6S7 

so       104a 

14       a26 

3« 

6xx 

..^ 

996 

xa    z8z 

89       566     46 

95" 

xo            X36 

2>bbb 

1 150 

F'bb  336 

i4«bb722 

C'b 

1 108 

Eb 

294 

G|    680 

B,    io66D,S 

252 

F,  J  8     63 J 

•        X155 

G*bbb  448 

X9         340 

36       7a5 

53= 

l?^b 

0  ZZ09 
406 

17 

894 

34    679 

51    X064 

«« 

849 

3a         634 
B,t      I 136 

B»bbb834 

D«bb  20 

iib 

792 

a,  I 178 

S,      364'<?,« 

750 

«4 

0*bbb 

«1 
946 

41       838 

5         »3 

aa 

408 

39 

79a 

3  "77 

ao       362     37 

747 

z          XZ33 

^4«        434 

£;>bbbi32G«bb5x8 

B«bb 

904'Db 

90 

F,    476\A^      862,C,« 

48 

46 

951 

xo         136     87       sai 

44 

906      8 

91 

as     47Sj    4a       860 

6 

45 

83            430 

-126 

"•94            "•63 

— 

31 

0 

+  •31 

+  •63 

+  •< 

H 

+  I'26 

I 

2                   3 

i 

1 

5 

6 

7 

8 

9 

-•32 
-•27 

-•23 

-•18  IT 
•14 

•09 

-05 

o 

+  •05 

+•09 

+•14 

+  i8 

f 

+  •23 


This  is  the  first  table  of  modulations  adapted  to  Just  Intonation  that  has  been  constructed. 
But  .the  table  in  (Gottfried  Weber's  Versuch  Hner  geordneten  Theorie  der  Tonaeizkunst  (Attempt 
%i  systematic  theory  of  musical  composition,  1830-2,  vol.  ii.,  §  180,  p.  86),  although  only 
Gbdapted  to  equal  temperament,  was  of  much  assistance  to  me. 

19.  Constniction  of  the  Duodenarium,  The  arrangement  is  that  of  all  the 
previous  schemes,  proceeding  from  bottom  to  top  by  intervals  of  a  Fifth  702  cents 
(or  from  top  to  bottom  by  intervals  of  a  Fourth  498  cents),  and  from  left  to  right  by 
intervals  of  a  major  Third  386  cents  (or  from  right  to  left  by  intervals  of  a  minor 
Bixth  814  cents).  The  number  written  against  any  note  shews  the  cyclic  intervals 
of  the  note  from  (7,  when  all  are  reduced  to  the  same  Octave,  see  App.  XX.  sect.  A. 
art.  24,  xiii.  p.  4376'. 

But  as  a  Fifth  is  701*955  cents,  and  a  major  Third  386-314,  errors  of  accumulation  occur, 
and  hence  the  cyclic  numbers  require  corrections  if  the  precise  numbers  are  wanted;  apply 
those  given  at  top  or  bottom  of  the  column,  or  at  either  end  of  the  line  containing  the  number. 
Thus  F28  S682  has  the  column  correction  +  -63,  and  the  line  correction --23,  and  its  true  dis- 
tance from  C  is  therefore  682-4  cents.  On  referring  to  the  name  of  the  note  in  the  Table, 
Beet.  A.  art.  28,  p.  440,  the  precise  number  of  cents  to  one  place  of  decimals,  the  logarithm  and 
pitch  of  the  note  will  be  found  in  addition. 

The  interval  between  any  two  notes,  reduced  to  the  same  Octave,  is  the  dif- 
ference of  the  number  of  cyclic  cents  assigned ;  corrected  if  required.  The  number 
of  the  note  by  which  the  just  note  would  be  represented  in  Bosanquet*s  cycle  of 
53,  is  added  in  smaller  figures  under  the  just  note,  and  the  nearest  whole  number 
of  cents  is  annexed.    Referring  to  that  number  in  the  Table  in  sect.  A.  art.  27, 

Digitized  by  V^OOQ IC 


1[ 


4^4' 


ADDITIONS  BY  THE  TRANSLATOR. 


APP.  XX. 


p.  439&,  the  name  given  by  Mr.  Bosanquet,  the  precise  number  of  cents,  the  loga- 
rithm and  the  pitch  will  be  found. 

20.  Jvst  Intervals  Bediiced  to  Steps  of  Fifths  and  Major  Thirds,  On  account 
of  the  construction  by  Fifths  and  major  Thirds,  we  can  proceed  from  any  note  to 
any  other  by  taking  a  certain  number  of  Fifths  up  or  down,  and  then  ms^'or 
Thirds  up  or  down,  and  reducing  to  the  same  Octave.  See  suprii,  sect.  D.  arts. 
7  and  9,  p.  452&^  c,  where  the  process  is  described. 


21.  The  Column  of  Fifths. 


The  central  column  of  Fifths  has  no  sape- 
nor  or  inferior  indexes.  The  superior  indexes 
*,*,*,*  to  the  left  not  only  serve  to  distinguish 
the  columns,  but  indicate  that  the  note  bear- 
ing it  is  I,  2,  3,  4  commas  of  22  cents  sharper 
I  or  higher  than  the  note  of  the  same  name  in 
'  the  column  of  Fifths.  Thus  Dbin  the  oolumn 
of  Fifths  has  90  cents,  D*bhas  therefore  90  + 
3  X  22  =  1 56  cents  as  there  marked.  The  in- 
ferior indexes  p  «,  ti  4  to  the  right  also  not 
merely  distinguish  &e  columns,  but  indicate 


that  the  notes  are  i,  2, 3,  4  commas  flatter  or 
lower  than  a  note  of  the  same  in  the  oolmnn 
of  Fifths.  Thus  Ct  has  1 14  cents,  but  C^  has 
114-3x22  =  48  cents.  It  is  thus  quite  easy 
to  continue  the  line  of  Fifths  up  at  least  to 
-DS  S  8  546  from  the  table  by  adding  the  ap- 
propriate number  of  commas,  thus  Z>8  8t 
=  ^4««« +45<22  =  458t88-546  cents  and 
down  to  Cbbb  858  by  subtracting  the 
as  shewn  by  C^bbb-4x  22  =  946-88=858. 


22.  Limits  of  the  Ihiodenarium. 


These  were  determined  thus :  The  central 
dark  oblong  is  the  duodene  of  C.  Within 
the  next  cUkrk  oblong  are  all  the  duodenes 
which  have  at  least  one  note  in  common  with 
the  duodene  of  C  The  extremes  are  the  duo- 
denes  of  D^b  (with  the  note  B»b),  of  A^bb 
(with  the  note  i>>b),  of  E^l  (with  the  note 
FyU ),  and  of  B^  (with  the  note  ^1,).  Then  the 
outer  black  oblong  contains  all  Uie  duodenes 
whose  roots  are   notes  in  the   intermediate 


black  oblong.  Supposing  the  original  dno* 
dene,  then,  to  be  one  which  had  its  root  in 
the  duodene  of  C  (which  may  always  be  con- 
sidered as  the  case),  the  limits  allow  of  modu- 
lation into  any  duodene  containing  that  note, 
and  thence  into  duodenes  which  have  at  least 
one  note  in  common  with  the  last  named.  We 
thus  obtain  9  x  13=  117  notes,  forming  7  x  10 
=  70  duodenes. 


23.  Introdnction  of  the  Seventh  and  Seventeenth  Harmonics. 


If  it  is  desired  to  proceed  beyond  tertian 
to  septimal  harmony  to  inftroduce  the  har- 
monic form  of  the  phord  of  the  dominant 
Seventh,  with  the  ratios  4:5:6:7  as  Mr. 
Poole  has  done  (see  sect.  F.  No.  7),  or  even 
to  septendecimal  harmony  to  introduce  the 
harmonic  form  of  the  chord  of  the  minor 
Ninth  8  :  10  :  12  :  14  :  17  (see  p.  346c,  note  ♦), 
the  number  of  the  notes  will  be  nearly  tripled. 
Taking  the  root  of  the  chord  as  C,  to  each 
minor  Seventh  Bbwe  should  have  to  add  'Pb, 
which  is  27  cyclic  cents  flatter  than  this 
minor  Seventh  Bb  (as  shewn  in  the  duo- 
denary arrangement  of  Mr.  Poole's  notes, 
I  sect.  F.  No.  7),  and  to  each  minor  Ninth  as 
i>ib  we  must  add  *^i>*bf  which  is  seven  cyclic 
cents  flatter  than  this  minor  Ninth.  The  cents 
in  the  tertian  chord  of  the  minor  Ninth  C  JB7, 
G  Bb  -D'b  are  o,  386,  702,  996,  1200+ 112. 
Hence  the  cents  in  the  harmonic  septimal 
chord  of  tiie  dominant  Seventh,  or  C  E^  G 
*Bb,  are  o,  386,  702,  969,  and  the  cents  in  the 
septendeeimal  chord  of  the  minor  Ninth,  or 


C  E,G  'Bb"i>»b,  are  o,  386,  702,  969,  1200 
+ 105.  This  form  can  be  played  in  all  its 
inversions  on  the  Harmonica),  see  sect.  P. 
No.  I.  If  the  root  be  omitted  in  the  cJiord 
of  the  minor  Ninth,  we  obtain  the  chord  of 
the  diminished  Seventh,  which  in  its  har- 
monic form  is  10 :  12  :  14  :  17,  or  E^  G  ^Bh 
*'jD'b,  in  cents  o,  316,  583,  919,  which  can  also 
be  played  on  the  Harmonical  in  all  its  inver- 
sions. In  Mr.  Bosanquet*B  cyde  of  53,  the 
chord  of  the  dominant  Seventh  is  played  by 
the  degrees  4,  21,  35,  47,  or  cents  o,  385,  702, 
974,  of  which  the  last  note  is  5  cents  too 
sharp,  but  the  effect  is  good.  The  chord  of 
the  diminished  Seventh  must  be  played  bj 
degrees  4,  18,  30.  45,  or  cents  o,  317,  589.  929. 
the  last  of  which  is  10  cents  too  sharp,  and 
the  result  would  not  be  improved  by  taking  it 
one  degree  or  23  cents  flatter.  Altogether  it 
is  only  a  slight  improvement  on  the  imitatioa 
of  the  tertian  form,  degrees  4,  18,  31,  45,  or 
cents  o,  317,  611,  929. 


24.  Need  of  Beduction  of  the  Number  of  Just  Tones, 


Of  course  it  is  quite  out  of  the  question 
that  any  attempt  should  be  made  to  deal  with 
such  numbers  of  tones  differing  often  by  only 
2  cents  from  each  other.  No  ear  could  appre- 
ciate tiie  multitude  of  distinctions.  No  in- 
strument, even  if  once  correctly  toned,  would 
keep  its  intonation  sufficiently  well  to  preserve 


such  niceties.  No  keyboard  could  be  invented 
for  playing  the  notes  even  if  they  eooid  bs 
tuned,  although,  as  will  be  seen  in  art.  26,  it 
is  very  easy  to  mark  a  piece  of  otdinazy 
music  so  as  to  indicate  the  piedse  notes  to 
be  struck.  Hence  some  compromise  is  needed, 
such  as  the  following. 


Digitized  by  VjOOQlC 


SECT.  E. 


MUSICAL  DUODENES. 


46.«; 


25.  The  Omissicni  of  the  Skhisma.     UneqiiaUy  Jiist  Intonation. 
The  Cycle  of  53. 


The  first  compromise  is  to  consider  all 
tones  differing  by  2  cents  (a  skhisma)  as  iden- 
tical. The  dotted  lines  in  the  Duodenarium 
inclose  7  x  8  =  56  tones  which  differ  from  each 
other  by  more  than  2  cents.  Any  note  in  the 
line  just  above  the  upper  dotted  line  differs 
only  by  2  cents  from  the  note  just  above  the 
lower  dotted  line  in  the  preceding  column. 
We  may  proceed  then  by  perfect  Fifths  of 
702  cents  up  from  D3S  252,  the  extreme  note 
in  the  right-hand  bottom  comer  of  this  oblong, 
to  D,t  1 366,  at  the  top,  and  thence  by  an  im- 
perfect Fifth  of  700  cents  (the  same  as  in  the 
equally  tempered  scale)  to  B^  1066  at  the 
bottom  of  the  next  column  to  the  left.  Then 
again  we  may  go  by  perfect  Fifths  to  Bjt  1 180, 
and  then  by  another  Fifth  of  700  cents  to  G, 
680  J=  1180  +  700- 1200)  and  so  on  till  we 
had  by  these  alternating  Fifths  of  702  and 
7cx>  cents  reached  the  56th  note  and  55th 
Fifth  F*b  450.  In  this  way,  at  the  53rd  Fifth 
or  54th  note  we  should  have  reached  E*[>b 
246,  over  which  a  short  line  is  drawn.  Now 
this  is  lower  than  the  initial  note  D^  252  by 
only  6  cents.    Hence  if  on  the  three  last  occa- 

26.  The  Duodenal,  The  duodenal  is  the  letter  name  of  the  root  of  any 
dnodene.  By  placing  it  over  any  note  or  chord  we  indicate  that  that  note  and  all 
which  follow  till  a  new  dttod^nal  is  giveii  are  to  have  such  values  only  as  tJiey 
would  have  in  the  duodene  of  which  the  tone  indicated  by  the  duodenal  is  the  root. 
This  prevents  all  ambiguity  by  restoring  in  fsbci  the  notation  of  commas  higher  or 
lower,  which  alone  is  wanting  for  the  representation  of  tertian  harmony  in  the 
ordinary  staff  notation.  If  the  7th  and  17th  harmonics  have  to  be  introduced  they  % 
will  have  sloping  lines  placed  before  them  as  in  chord  1 7  below.  The  examples 
given  are  not  intended  as  specimens  of  desirable  harmony,  but  of  the  means  of 
representing  differences  of  just  intonation.  The  first  16  chords  are  from  God  save 
the  Queen  ;  the  four  last  are  merely  examples  of  notation. 


sions  where  Fifths  of  700  cents  were  to  have 
been  used,  we  had  taJcen  the  perfect  Fifths 
of  702  cents  we  should  have  made  C*b^  mo 
cents,  ii*bb»726  cents  and  ^bb»342  cents, 
and  consequently  E*\)b  252  cents.  This  would 
have  become  identical  with  the  starting  note 
Dji  252.  This  mode  of  tuning,  which  if  accu- 
rately executed  no  ear  could  distinguish  from 
just  intonation,  forms  the  unequally  jtist  in- 
tonation  mentioned  in  sect.  A.  art.  19,  p.  435c. 
It  is  also  the  foundation  of  substituting  for 
the  perfect  Fifth  another  of  31  x  1200-7-53  = 
701-886  cents,  80  that  on  repeating  it  53  times, 
and  deducting  31  Octaves  we  should  come  back 
to  the  starting  note.  And  this  gives  the  cycle  %' 
of  53  already  described,  sect.  A.  arts.  22  and  27, 
to  which  reference  is  made  on  the  Duoden- 
arium itself,  shewing  exactly  the  mode  in 
which  it  can  be  substituted  for  Just  Intona- 
tion without  perceptible  injury  to  the  har- 
monic effect.  For  this  and  other  less  happy 
but  more  handy  attempts,  see  sect.  A.  The 
mode  of  fingering  this  cycle  is  explained  infr4, 
sect.  F.  Nos.  8  and  9,  and  of  tuning  it  in  sect. 
G.  arts.  19  and  20. 


C        G    C 

I     I     I,    I 


123     4     56     7    S    9      10    II  12    13  14    15      16 


17  18 


19    20 


The  chords  are  numbered  for  convenience 
of  reference,  and  only  the  treble  is  given  for 
brevity.  When  the  bass  is  added,  the  duo- 
denal should  be  repeated  in  the  bass  or  merely 
placed  between  treble  and  bass.  Observe  the 
chords  3,  9,  13,  which  introduce  the  ambi- 
guous chord  on  the  second.  The  duodenal 
C  over  chord  i  shews  that  we  begin  in  the 
duodene  of  c,  so  that  the  first  chord  is  e'  316 
g^  498  e'.  But  G  over  chord  3  shews  that  there 
is  a  dnodenation  into  the  dominant,  and  that 
the  chord  is  the  true  minor  /''  386  a'  498  d"  and 
not  the  dissonant  chord  of  tiie  added  Sixth 
/  386  a/  520  d".  The  d"  must  be  retained  for 
the  voice  to  descend  by  a  perfect  minor  Third 
to  6/  in  chord  4,  and  be  the  true  Octave  of  d' 
in  that  chord  wliere  C  shews  that  the  duodene 
of  C  is  again  reached.  Hence  it  is  not  allow- 
able to  tak^  chord  3  in  the  dnodene  of  F  as 
/386  a/  498  dy".  The  following  chords  5,  6,  7, 
8  are  also  in  the  duodene  of  C,  as  there  is  no 
change  of  duodenal.  But  chord  0  is  in  the 
duodene  of  F,  because  a/  is  retained  from 


chord  8,  and  d"j  f*  must  harmonise  with  it. 
In  chord  10  the  duodene  of  C  is  again  reached. 
As  purposely  written  in  this  example  chord  13 
is  the  dissonant  added  Sixth  ^386  a,'  520  d'\  ^ 
which  is  resolved  on  chord  14,  but  the  reten- 
tion of  a{  would  make  it  more  natural  to  take 
the  duodene  of  F,  as  /'386  a,'  498  d"  and  then 
return  immediately  to  the  duodene  of  C.  In 
chord  15,  d'267  ^f  2^1  ^  386  6/  the  method  is 
shewn  by  which  the  septimal  7  is  indicated. 
The  duodenal  C  would  make  /  without  the 
mark  before  it,  to  be  true  Fourth  of  the  root 
c.  But  this  Fourth  is  27  cents  too  sharp  for 
the  7th  harmonic  of  the  dominant  g,  and 
hence  the  line  sloping  down  to  the  right  in- 
dicates that  the  Fourth  has  to  be  taken  27 
cents  flatter  in  s^timal  harmony.  In  ordi- 
nary tertian  harmony  as  indicated  by  the  duo- 
denal only,  the  Fourth  would  remain  unaltered. 
In  chord  17,  the  duodenal  C  would  shew  that 
the  a'b  must  be  a^'b,  the  minor  Sixth  Of  the 
root  c,  or  a  diatonic  Semitone  above  th^  domi- 
nant g.    But  this  is  7  cents  too  sharp  for  the 

Digitized  by  V560gle 


466  ADDITIONS  BY  THE   TRANSLATOR.  app.  xx. 

17th  harmonic  of  the  dominant,  and  henoe  served  if  this  minor  Sixth  werieoBedin  place 
the  line  sloping  down  to  the  right  indicates  of  the  17th  haimonic  provided  only  the  7th 
that  it  is  to  be  7  cents  flatter.  The  sloping  harmonic  of  the  dominant  were  retained, 
line,  therefore,  indicates  different  degrees  of  Equal  temperament  of  course  not  recognis* 
flattening  according  as  it  is  applied  to  the  ing  the  difference  of  a  comma,  so  far  as  sound 
Fourth  or  minor  Sixth  of  the  root  expressed  is  concerned,  retains  the  same  tempered  duo- 
by  the  duodenal.  If,  therefore,  we  wished  to  dene  throughout,  although  there  is  a  difference 
have  the  chord  «,'  316  fir'267  '6'b336  *'d*"b  we  in  writing  it,  as  would  be  shewn  in  the  Duo- 
must  write  the  duodenal  as  F  to  get  the  right  denarium  (p.  463)  if  the  indices  were  omitted, 
intonation,  as  in  chord  19.  Since  the  17th  Such  an  omission  reduces  the  Duodenarium 
harmonic  of  the  dominant  is  so  nearly  the  to  a  table  of  modulations  in  any  temperament 
minor  Sixth  of  the  root,  and  the  chord  is  which  neglects  the  comma, 
dissonant,  much  of  the  effect  would  be  pre- 

SECTION  F. 

m  BZPBBIXEMTAI*  ZNSTBUMBNT8  TOB  EXHIBITIMO  THS   BFFECTS  OV  JUST   OTTONATIOM. 

(See  notes  pp.  6,  17,  217,  218,  222,  256,  329,  and  346.) 

No.  No. 

Introduction,  p.  466.  5.  Rev.  H.  Listen's  Organ,  p.  473. 

1.  The  Harmonical,  p.  466.  6.  General  Thompson's  Organ,  p.  473. 

2.  The  Just  Harmonium,  p.  470.  7.  Mr.  H.  W.  Poole's  Organ,  p.  474. 

3.  The  Just  English  Concertina,  p.  470.  8.  Mr.  Bosanquet's  Generalised  Fingerboard 

4.  BIr.  Colin  Brown's  Voice  Harmoninm,  p.  and  Harmonium,  p.  479. 

470.  9.  Mr.  Paul  White's  Hiumon,  p.  481. 

Inteoduction. 

At  the  present  day  ordinary  masical  instruments  are  intended  to  be  toned  in 
accordance  with  equal  temperament  (see  pp.  313a,  432^,  art.  10 ;  4366,  art.  22,  i. ; 
437c,  art.  25  ;  sect.  G.  arts.  11  and  following).  The  English  concertina,  which  has 
14  keys  for  the  Octave,  is  still  usually  tuned  in  the  older  Meantone  temperament 

H  (P-  433^»  art*  ^6»  ^^^  ^^^'  ^*  ^'  ^^)'  ^^*  neither  system  gives  the  only  inter^ 
which  will  allow  chords  in  the  middle  part  of  the  scale  to  be  played  without  giving 
rise  to  beats.  In  order,  then,  that  the  ear  may  learn  what  is  the  meaning  of '  just 
intonation,'  it  is  necessary  for  it  to  have  special  instruments,  or  at  least  instru- 
ments specially  tuned.  Prof.  Helmholtz  has  for  this  purpose  invented  a  tuning 
for  an  harmonium  with  two  rows  of  ordinary  keys,  explained  on  pp.  31 62)  to  320a. 
Others,  as  Colin  Brown,  Liston,  Poole,  and  Perronet  Thompson,  have  invented 
harmoniums  or  organs  with  novel  fingerboards ;  and  others,  as  Bosanquet  and 
J.  P.  White,  have  invented  means  for  using  the  division  of  the  Octave  into  53  parts, 
which,  as  is  seen  in  sect.  E.,  p.  463,  is  practically  almost  identical  with  just  intona- 
tion. A  brief  account  of  these  instruments  (with  the  exception  of  Prof.  Helm- 
holtz's,  which  is  fully  described  in  the  text)  will  here  be  given.  But  none  of  them 
meet  the  wants  of  the  student.  They  are  all  too  expensive  and  require  so  much 
special  education  to  use,  that  (with  the  exception  of  Mr.  Colin  Brown's)  they  have 
remained  musical  curiosities,  some  of  them  entirely  unique.     But  there  are  two 

^  instruments  which  are  cheap  and  which  can  be  tuned  so  as  to  illustrate  almost 
every  point  of  theory,  though  they  of  course  remain  experimental  instruments 
intended  only  to  shew  the  nature  of  musical  intervals,  chords,  and  scales,  and  not 
to  play  pieces  of  music  except  especially  composed  exercises.  These  two  I  shall 
take  first.  They  are  a  specially  tuned  harmonium  and  English  concertina.  Beed 
instruments  are  far  the  best  for  experiments,  because  they  give  sustained  notes 
possessing  a  large  number  of  powerfid  upper  partial  tones,  so  that  any  deviations 
from  just  intonation  are  extremely  conspicuous,  painfully  evident  indeed  on  any 
harmonium  tuned  in  equal  temperament. 

I.  The  Harmonical. 

The  scale  of  the  Harmonical  and  the  number  of  vibrations  for  every  note  in  &• 
first  four  octaves  will  be  found  on  p.  17,  note.  The  instrument  has  been  con^ 
stantly  referred  to  in  the  Translator's  notes  to  the  preceding  pages.  It  is  an 
harmonium  with  one  row  of  vibrators  extending  over  five  octaves.  The  toning  of 
tiie  fifth  octave  wiU  be  explained  further  on. 


Digitized  by 


Google 


SECT.  F. 


EXPERIMENTAL  JUST  INTONATION. 


467 


Any  one  baying  such  an  hannoniun  of 
Ifessra.  Moore  A  Moore,  pianoforte  and  har- 
monium makers,  104  and  105  Bishopsgate 
Street,  London,  for  165s.  net,  may  have  it 
tuned  as  an  Hannonical,  by  my  forks,  and 
provided  with  an  *harmonical  bar'  as  pre- 
sently explained,  both  without  extra  charge. 
I  am  sure  that  all  musical  students,  as  well 
as  myself,  must  feel  greatly  indebted  to  this 
firm,  who  at  the  instance  of  Mr.  H.  Keatley 
Moore,  Mus.  B.,  a  student  of  the  first  edition 
of  this  work,  have  so  kindly  undertaken  to 
furnish  this  almost  indispensable  aid  to  the 
Btudy  of  music  on  Hehnholtz's  principles  at 
Buch  a  very  moderate  cost. 

On  the  first  four  octaves  this  instrument 
contains  all  the  10  notes  of  the  Decad  of  C 


(p.  459&),  and  hence  all  its  chords  (p.  459c), 
and  allows  of  playing  and  harmonising  all  the 
56  trichordal  scales  (p.  460)  contained  in  that 
decad.  Its  10  notes,  C  D  E'b  E^  F  O  A^\)  A^ 
B'b  ^1,  are  placed  on  their  usual  digitals. 
Hence  so  far  there  is  no  new  fingering  to 
learn.  The  remaining  two  digitals  are  em- 
ployed to  furnish  two  notes  of  great  theoreti- 
cal importance,  the  grave  Second  D,,  which  is 
of  course  placed  on  the  Db  or  C%  digital,  and 
the  natural  or  harmonic  Seventh  'Bb,  which 
had  to  be  placed,  rather  out  of  order,  on  the 
6  b  or  ^  digital,  the  only  one  at  liberty. 
Hence,  using  small  letters  to  represent  the 
short  black  keys,  the  keyboard  for  each  of  the 
first  four  octaves  is 


C  D  E^ 

vib.  in  two-foot  Octave  264  { "^^*  ^o;  ^'^  330        352 


'6b        a*b        6*b 
F  G         A,  B^  C 

46a  49»f  475J 

396        440        495        528 


and  its  scheme  in  the  Decad  form  with  the 
two  additional  notes  is 


B'b 
E'\> 
A'b 


D 
O 
C 
F 


'Bb 


^1 


In  this  form  (...)  in  the  second  column 
indicates  the  absence  of  Bb,  and  ^Bb  forms  a 
column  by  itself.  The  scheme  is  seen  filled 
up  on  p.  474c,  d.  The  addition  of  ^Bb  gives 
an  opportunity  of  playing  the  first  sixteen 
harmonics  of  C  with  the  exception  of  the 
I  ith  and  13th  (whence  the  name  Harmonical), 
thus: 


Note  C      c       g      c'      e^'     g'     '6'b    c"     d"     c/' 

Harmonic    123456789       10 


12        14       15        16 


By  means  of  the  *harmonical  bar'  pro- 
vided with  the  instrument,  these  harmonics, 
except  the  7th  and  I4ih,  can  be  pressed  down 
at  the  same  time,  and  then  the  7th  and  14th, 
being  on  short  keys,  can  be  added  with  the 
fingers  of  the  hands  which  press  down  the  bar. 
The  pegs  which  press  the  notes  are  arranged 
on  different  lines,  so  that  the  first  8  harmonics 
can  be  played  by  themselves,  and  then  the 
effect  of  adding  the  higher  Octave  can  be  tried. 
It  is  thus  possible  to  play  the  harmonics 
simultaneously  with  or  without  the  7th  and 
14th,  and  thus  to  estimate  their  presumed 
dissonant  effect.  To  my  own  feeling  these' 
harmonics  greatly  enrich  and  improve  the 
quality  of  the  very  compound  tone  produced. 

It  is  evident  therefore  that  the  effects  of 
all  the  intervals  depending  on  the  numbers 
I  to  16  (omitting  ii  and  13)  can  be  immedi- 
ately produced,  and  hence  all  the  intervals  on 
p.  2126,  c,  induding  the  septimal  intervals,  aris- 
ing from  'Bb,  which  are  of  special  importance 
and  interest  because  they  can  be  so  rarely 
heard. 

The  existence  of  higher  upper  partials  of 
the  low  notes  can  easily  be  made  evident  by 
beats.  If  we  press  down  one  of  the  digitals 
for  the  shortest  distance  that  will  allow  the 
note  to  sound  at  all,  we  flatten  it  slightly,  and 
hence  put  it  out  of  tune.  Keeping  then  C 
sounding  fully,  and  slightly  flattening  its  har- 
monics, one  by  one  in  tills  way  (indicated  by  a 
prefixed  grave  accent)  we  easily  obtain  the 
beats  from  Ccy  C%  Ccf,  C'e,',  Cg\  C"6'b, 
CTc".  Cd",  CTe/',  CTg",  CTb^b,  Cl^\  Cc"\ 
making  evident  the  existence  of  13  out  of  15 
of  the  upper  partials  of  C  In  the  same  way 
by  slightly  flattening  the  upper  or  lower  notes 
of  any  of  the  consonant  intervals,  as  e  :  (7,  we 


IT 


can  produce  the  beats  which  shew  that  the 
consonance  has  been  disturbed.  These  are 
some  of  the  most  striking  illustrations  of 
Helmholtz's  discoveries. 

Beats  between  the  primes  of  two  notes  are 
well  shewn  by  DDp  <W„  d'd{,  d"d^\  which 
should  beat  about  9,  18,  37,  73  times  in 
10  seconds,  the  number  of  beats  doubling  for 
each  ascent  of  an  Octave.  The  very  impure 
character  of  the  beats  of  i)/>„  arising  from 
our  hearing  at  the  same  time  the  beats  of  the 
upper  pairs  of  notes  as  partials,  is  instructive. 
We  can  also  hear  the  beats  faJl  given  for  10 
seconds  and  fractions  omittea)  in  D£>b  50, 
'Bb  B>b  33t  ^'b  E^  33,  il'b  A,  44,  B»b  B,  50, 
but  the  higher  Octaves  of  these  notes  beat  too 
rapidly  to  be  counted. 

Combinational  tones  are  easily  heard.  Any 
two  consecutive  harmonics  of  C  give  C,  and 
by  sounding  two  of  them  strongly  and  slightly  % 
flattening  the  C,  the  beats  of  this  flattened 
^C  with  the  combinational  tone  may  be  heard, 
but  much  care  and  attention  are  necessary  for 
this  purpose.  On  pUying  6,"  c'"  the  rattle  of 
the  66  beats  in  a  second  may  be  heard,  as 
well  as  the  combinational  C  of  66  vib.  Simi- 
larly  for  6,"  and  6"'b  the  rattle  of  the  39-6 
beats  in  a  second,  and  also  the  deep  combina- 
tional tone  ^'^b  of  39-6  vib.  And  if  all  three 
keys  6»"b,  6,",  and  c"  be  held  down  together, 
the  low-pitched  beat  of  the  two  combinational 
tones  may  also  be  heard  with  proper  care  and 
attention.  If  we  play  <2/'/'  we  have  a  beat- 
note  of  1 17*3  vib.,  very  nearly  B>b.  If  we  play 
d"/'  we  have  the  beat-note  il|  of  1 10  vib.  If  we 
play  all  three  together  the  two  beat-notes  beat 
73*3  times  in  10  seconds.  This  must  be  care- 
fully listened  for,  but  the  beats  being  so  much 
lower  in  pitch  cannot  be  confused  with  the 


Digitized  by  V^tibgie 


468 


ADDITIONS  BY  THE   TEANSLATOR, 


APP.  XX. 


higher  beats  of  d/'  d",  although  their  fre- 
quency is  the  same. 

All  the  forms  of  the  major  and  minor 
triads  and  tetrads  on  pp.  2  i  86  to  224a  oan  be 
played  and  appreciated,  and  in  many  cases 
the  combinational  tones  can  be  played  as  sub- 
stantive notes  with  them ;  see  my  footnotes  to 
these  pages. 

The  e£Pect  of  the  analyses  of  dyads  in  my 
footnotes  on  pp.  188  to  191  can  all  be  studied, 
and  much  of  the  diagrams  on  pp.  193  and  333 
can  be  verified. 

Most  of  the  old  Greek  tetrachords  on 
p.  263^2'  can  be  played  as  there  pointed  out. 

The  analysis  of  scales  on  pp.  274-278  can 
be  illustrated. 

The  discords  in  Chap.  XVII.  oan  be  mostly 
^  illustrated,  as  pointed  out  in  my  footnotes. 

As  shewn  by  the  table  on  p.  17,  note,  the 
intervals  80  :  81,  or  comma,  the  minor  Tone 
9  :  10,  and  major  Tone  8  :  9,  the  diatonic 
Semitone  15  :  16,  and  small  Semitone  24  :  25, 
and  other  important  intervals,  can  all  be  illus- 
trated. Again,  'Bb  :  £'b  =  3S  :  36  is  49  cents, 
and  hence  almost  precisely  a  quarter  of  a 
Tone  or  50  cents,  and  A^  :  'Bb  =  20  :  21  is  85 
cents,  or  very  nearly  the  Pythagorean  Limma 
of  90  cents.  The  imperfect  Fifth  of  just  in- 
tonation Z) :  iip  or  680  cents,  may  be  con- 
trasted with  the  perfect  Fifth  D| :  il„  or  702 
cents.  The  Pythagorean  minor  Third  D  :  F^ 
or  294  cents,  can  be  contrasted  with  the  just 
minor  Third  D^  :  J?',-or  316  cents. 

But  it  is  also  necessary  to  note  what  the 
Harmonical  cannot  do.  It  has  no  Pythagorean 
conmia,  and  no  Pythagorean  major  Third,  nor 
c|  can  it  play  a  Pythagorean  scale.  It  cannot 
play  the  chord  of  the  extreme  sharp  Sixth, 
nor  can  it  modulate  into  the  dominant  or  sub- 
dominant,  or  relative  minor  (except  in  the 
descending  form),  but  it  can  distinguish 
/  386  «!  520  <2,  the  chord  of  the  added  Sixth, 
from  the  minor  chord/  386  a,  498  <2„  and  oan 
modulate  from  C  major  to  C  minor. 

It  is  also  able  to  play  Mr.  Poole's  dichordal 
scale  F  A  C,C  E  O'Bb  D  with  the  peculiar 
minor  chord  O  :  'Bb  :  D  =  6  :  7  :  9,  and  the 
full  natural  chord  of  the  major  Ninth. 

Method  of  Tutiing,  To  be  sure  about  the 
pitches,  I  tuned  c"528,  a'440,  a"b422-4, 
'6'b462  on  forks  with  great  accuracy,  by  means 
of  my  tuning-forks  mentioned  on  p.  4466'.  I 
tuned  also  a  second  set  of  forks  each  two 
beats  flatter  than  the  above,  which  I  found 
very  useful  in  determining  the  accuracy  of  the 
m  tuning  by  unisons.    In  fact  the  note  of  the 


reed  is  so  much  more  powerful  than  that  of  the 
fork,  that  the  latter  was  quite  drowned  when 
near  the  unison,  so  that  the  pitch  could  not 
be  determined  within  3  to  5  beats  in  10 
seconds,  and  this  difficulty  was  entirely  obvi- 
ated by  the  flat  forks.  After  these  notes  then 
had  been  tuned  on  the  Harmonical,  the  rest  of 
the  notes  in  the  two-foot  Octave  were  tuned  by 
Fifths  or  Fourths,  namely  flrst  a»'b  to  e^% 
e»'b  to  6»'b,  secondly  c"  to  ^,  gf'  to  d'\  cT  to/, 
thirdly  a,'  to  «/',  «,"  to  fe/,  and  a,'  to  d/.  The 
other  notes  were  obtained  by  Octaves.  The 
verification  is  by  the  perfect  major  chords 
FA^C,  CEfi,  GB,D,  A'bCE^b,  JT'bOB'b; 
the  perfect  minor  chord  D^FA^  and  the  per- 
fect chord  of  the  harmonic  Seventh  CEfi^B)>, 
all  without  beats  in  the  two-foot  Octave. 

Pitch.  The  pitch  (^'528  was  choeen  to 
agree  with  the  pitch  adopted  by  Prof.  Helm- 
holtz  in  the  text;  a'440  was  the  pitch  pro- 
posed by  Scheibler ;  a''b422-4  is  within  -i  vib. 
of  the  pitch  of  HandePs  own  A  fork  422*5,  now 
in  the  possession  of  Bev.  G.  T.  Driffield,  Bector 
of  Old,  near  Northampton.  In  the  notes  not 
tuned  by  forks  there  may  be  a  very  slight  but 
not  perceptible  error,  so  that  the  Hannonieal 
presents  a  series  of  trustworthy  pitches. 

Eacercises,  Besides  numerous  short  airs, 
and  special  exercises,  the  following  pieces 
may  be  played  with  full  harmonies,  and  will 
serve  to  illustrate  the  meaning  of  just  intona- 
tion, especially  if  they  are  contrasted  with  the 
same  airs  immediately  afterwards  played  on 
an  ordinarily  tuned  harmonium. 

Ood  save  the  Queen  (in  C  major  with  its 
minor  chord  on  the  Second  of  the  scale,  alter- 
nating with  the  chord  of  the  added  Sixth). 

The  Heavens  are  telling  {C  major  with  the 
modulation  into  C  minor). 

OUyrious  Apollo  (altering  the  brief  modula- 
tion into  the  dominant). 

The  Old  Hundredth  (C  major). 

John  Anderson  (0  nunor). 

Adeste  Fideles  (avoiding  the  modulation 
into  the  dominant). 

Auld  Lang  Syne  (in  C  major). 

Dies  ir€Bt  in  part  (C  minor  modulating 
into  C  major). 

Leisej  leise  (the  prayer  in  Der  FreyschSti 
in  Poole's  dichordal  scale  FAfi,  CEfiB\>D, 
altering  the  harmonies  to  suit  the  new  scale). 

Crudel  perchA  {NoMMe  di  Figa/ro^  in  C 
minor,  altered,  but  preserving  the  burst  into 
C  major). 

Wanderer's  NachtUed  (Schubert). 

The  Manly  Heart  (ZauberflOU). 


So  much  relates  to  the  lower  four  Octaves  of  the  Harmonical,  which  suffice  to 
illustrate  all  the  principal  peculiarities  of  just  intonation.  Advantage  has  been 
taken  of  the  Fifth  or  6-inch  Octave  to  exhibit  some  of  the  higher  harmonics  of 
C  66,  and  to  give  a  complete  series  of  the  first  16  harmonics  of  C  132,  inclndhig 
the  nth  and  13th.    These  notes  are  as  follows : 


Harmonics 
Black  digitals 
White  digitals 
Pitch  numbers 


16 


17 


18       19 

»V"b 


20 


1056   1 122   1 188   1254   1320 


Of  course  with  such  high  pitches  there  has 
been  great  difficulty  in  tuning,  and  there  are 
probably  several  slight  errors,  but  none  that 
will  interfere  with  the  general  effect.  I  pro- 
ceeded thus.  The  harmonics  16,  18,  20,  28, 
^4>  30t  32  were  the  Octaves  of  harmonics  8,  9, 


22       28       24       25       26  29       30      33 

'l/"b           »a'"b  »6"b 

»»/"                g"f               »«te  I/**     (T 

1452   1848  1584   1650  1 716  1914    1980  21 13 

10,  14,  12,  15,  16  already  tuned  for  the  lower 
Octave.  Hence  only  17,  19,  22,  25,  26,  29  re- 
mained to  be  treated,  but  they  were  in  them- 
selves far  too  high  for  me  to  tune  forks  for. 
I  tuned  therefore  ^^d"b  with  561  vib.,  the  17th 
harmonic,  to  which  I  had  the  Octave  made  I7 


Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


SECT.  F. 


EXPERIMENTAL  JUST  INTONATION. 


469 


Messrs.  Valantine  A  Carr,  music  smiths,  of 
76  Milton  Street,  Sheffield.  Then  I  tuned 
»Vb3i3"S.  'V363*  '•a'b4i2-j,  »a'429,  *»6'b478-5 
all  harmonics  of  C„i6'$  and  hence  two  Octaves 
too  low.  From  these  Messrs.  Valantine  A  Carr 
made  me  forks  giving  the  Octaves  with  great 
accuracy,  and  afterwards  the  Octaves  of  these 
forks,  which  so  far  as  I  could  test  them  also 
appeared  accurate,  but  it  was  very  difficult  to 
form  an  accurate  judgment  of  the  pitch  of 
these  high  tuning-forks.  From  the  forks  thus 
made  the  remainder  of  the  fifth  octave  was 
tuned.  But  as  the  tone  of  the  reed  drowned 
that  of  the  fork«  I  had  here  also  a  second 
series  of  flatter  forks  constructed,  beating  twice 
in  a  second  with  the  former.  Of  course  I  have 
not  been  able  personally  to  check  the  tuning 
of  all  the  Harmonicals,  but  I  worked  with  the 
toner  at  first  and  saw  that  he  perfectly  well 
understood  what  was  to  be  done,  so  that  I  con- 
fidently hope  the  Harmonicals  he  turns  out 
will  answer  their  purpose.  One  of  them  was 
exhibited  in  the  International  Inventions 
Exhibition  of  1885,  Division  n..  Music. 

By  means  of  Uiis  fifth  octave  the  instru- 

10  :  12 :  14  :  17      =  «,"  :  gf"  : 
12 :  14  :  17  :  20     =   g'* : 
14 :  17  :  20 :  24      ^ 
17  :  20 :  24  :  28 

The  extreme  height  of  the  pitch  of  these  notes,  however,  will  prevent  a  due  appreciation  of 
these  chorda  as  compared  with  the  usual  forms,  which  can  only  be  played  at  a  lower  pitch  thus : 

20      «  d'      * 


ment  has  now  all  the  first  32  harmonics  of 
C66,  except  6,  namely  11,  13,  21,  23,  27,  31  ; 
and  has  all  the  first  16  harmonics  of  c  132  with- 
out exception.  There  are  additional  loose  pegs 
to  the  harmonical  bar,  which  can  be  inserted, 
in  order  to  play  all  these  16  harmonics  at  once, 
with  the  exception  of  the  7th  and  14th,  which, 
being  on  black  digitals,  most  be  struck  with 
the  finger  as  before. 

The  fifth  octave  therefore  gives  the 
trumpet  scale  8,  9,  10,  ii,  12,  13,  14,  15,  16, 
all,  with  exception  of  14,  on  the  white  digitals. 
These  give  the  peculiar  intervals  10  :  1 1  :=  165 
cents,  II  :  12^^151  cents,  12  :  13=139  cents, 
13  :  14  » 128  cents.  The  inharmonic  character 
of  these  intervals  is,  however,  not  well  brought 
out,  owing  to  the  weakness  of  the  upper  par- 
tials  in  this  region.  Other  intervals  of  interest  m 
are  the  approximations  to  the  tempered  Semi- 
tone, 16  :  17  s  105  cents,  17  :  18  =  99  cents. 

The  17th  harmonic  allows  of  playing  the 
harmonic  form  of  the  chord  of  the  diminished 
Seventh  in  its  direct  form  and  all  its  inver- 
sions as 

:  '6"b  :  "d'"b 
:  *6"b  :  "<i"'b  :  «,'" 
6"b  :  ''d'"b  :  «/"  :  a"' 
=    "d'"b  :  «/"  :  /"  :  'I/"b 


:  i4| :  17 

:  14I :  17 

14S :  17 


iv.20: 
7^:20: 


:/':a>'b:6/ 
24     -  /' :  a"b  :  W  :  ^" 
24:  28J  =  a"b:6/:d":/" 


of  which  only  the  first  shews  the  full  harshness  of  the  chord. 

It  is  thus  seen  that  the  Harmonical  is  the  only  instrument  yet  tuned  which 
brings  out  the  full  nature  of  just  intonation  for  the  7th  and  17th  harmonics. 

The  difficulty  in  tuning  the  Harmonical  without  forks  may  be  to  a  great 
extent  avoided  by  the  following  means,  which  will  enable  any  possessor  of  a  cheap 
harmonium  which  he  is  willing  to  sacrifice  as  an  experimental  instrument,  to  get 
it  tuned  by  a  professional  tuner.  It  will,  however,  be  necessary  to  give  up  the 
peculiar  arrangement  of  the  fifth  octave,  and  when  it  exists  on  any  harmonium 
to  have  it  tuned  simply  as  an  Octave  higher  than  the  fourth  octave. 


First  tune  the  11  notes  C  D^  D  E^\>  E^  F 
O  A^b  ^1  B*b  Bi  thus.  Take  C  to  the  exist- 
ing pitch  on  the  instrument.  Tune  the  Fifths 
cf  ig'/g^ :  d'\  /' :  c"  till  they  leave  no  trace  of 
beats.  Then  take  &'  :  e/'  and  a*'b  :  c''  to  be 
as  perfect  major  Thirds  without  beats  as  the 
tuner  can  make  them,  verifying  by  the  major 
chord  c'e,V»  M^d  minor  chord  /'a*'bc^  which 
should  both  be  without  beats.  The  combina- 
tional tones  (which  should  be  C  for  c"  :  e/'  and 
A^b  for  a"b  *.  c")  will  also  be  a  guide  to  the 
ear.  But  there  is  very  little  chance  of  perfect 
accuracy,  the  ears  of  tuners  having  been  spoiled 
by  the  sharp  major  Thirds  of  equal  tempera- 
ment. It  is  best  to  begin  by  tuning  these 
Thirds  decidedly  too  flat,  beating  10  or  20 
times  in  10  seconds,  and  then  gradually  to 
sharpen  tiU  the  beats  apparently  vanish.  The 
Thirds  may  thus  remain  very  slightly  flat,  like 
the  skhismic  Thirds  on  p.  2Sid\  and  they 
will  give  very  good  results.  The  point  is  to 
avoid  sharp  major  Thirds.  Then  tune  the 
Fifths  «/  :  6/,  a,  :  a/,  d,' :  o/,  and  a"b  :  e"% 
e^'b  :  6*'b»  the  necessary  Octaves  having  been 
previously  tuned.    Verify  by  the  major  chords 


ray\  c'e.'g^,  g^^d";  a'b  c^e'%  e^'b  g'b'X 
and  the  minor  chords  d/f^a^',  a/c"«,",  e/g'b' ; 
/'a»'b  c",  (/e"b  g',  g'b^'b  d'\  all  of  which  should 
be  perfect  without  sensible  beats.  Then  only 
'6  b  remains  to  be  tuned.  To  this  we  may  ap- 
proximate very  closely  thus.  In  the  first  place  «- 
it  is  49  cents,  or  say  a  quarter  of  a  Tone  (that 
is,  half  a  tempered  Semitone),  flatter  than  6'b 
which  has  already  been  tuned,  and  many  toners 
can  approximate  to  this  interval.  Next,  in  the 
lowest  octave  £'b  and  'Bb  will  beat.  If  the 
pitch  happens  to  be  ^"528,  then  the  beats  are 
33  in  10  seconds.  For  c"540,  which  is  sharp 
band  pitch,  the  beats  would  be  not  quite  34  in 
I  o  seconds.  For  c"5 1 8,  which  may  be  taken  as 
French  pitch,  the  beats  would  be  almost  ex- 
actly 32  in  10  seconds.  Hence  by  taking  them 
as  33  in  10  seconds  for  any  pitch,  the  tuner 
will  come  very  near  the  truth.  After  tuning 
the  Octaves,  he  will  verify  with  the  chord 
efe^'g'^Vbt  which  should  be  without  any  sen- 
sible beats  and  have  merely  a  slight  roughness. 
Even  a  rough  approximation  to  the  true  value 
of  ^Bbt  as  on  the  53  division  of  the  Octave, 
will  gratify  most  ears. 

Digitized  by  VjOOQlC 


£'t> 

2) 

£'b 

a 

B, 

^'b 

G 

Ex 

i^-b 

F 

^. 

470  ADDITIONS  BY  THE  TRANSLATOR.  app.  xx. 

2.  The  Just  Habmqnium. 

This  I  used  for  some  years.  The  'Bl>  is  sacrificed,  as  also  the  D,  ;  and  the  12 
notes  are  taken  as  in  the  margin,  F^  being  put  on  the  F%  digital.  F^ 
It  therefore  contains  the  duodene  of  C,  with  the  exception  of  i^ijj, 
and  with  the  addition  of  F^.  This  was  tuned  by  an  ordinary  tuner 
in  my  presence  in  two  hours  from  the  following  directions.  Make 
the  7  major  chords  CE^Q,  GB^D,  FA^C,  A^\^CE^\^,  E^\^GB% 
B^\}DF\  D^\}FA^\^  perfect  without  beats. 

This  gives  more  power  of  playing,  as  it  contains  the  decad  of  C  complete,  and 
hence  C  major  without  the  grave  second,  and  G  minor  in  all  its  forms,  with 
all  the  56  modes  ;  E^\}  major  with  the  grave  second  F,  and  A^\}  major  without  the 
grave  second.  But  the  harmonic  Seventh  ^-Eb,  and  grave  second  Di  are  much 
missed  in  C  major.  There  is  power  of  modulating  from  E^\}  major  to  its  sub- 
dominant  A^\}  major  (without  the  grave  second)  and  also  into  its  relative  minor  C 
IT  Hence  this  plan  of  tuning  has  many  advantages,  especially  in  being  easily  effected 
by  any  tuner  without  forks. 

3.  The  Just  English  Concbbtina, 

For  many  years  I  have  been  in  the  habit  of  making  my  experiments  on  one  of 
these  instruments  tuned  thus : 

Black  studs  Cjjj!    d    d^^   f\%    g^lf^    a     h^ 
White  studs  G       D,  e[    F       G      A^  B\} 

having  the  following  duodenary  arrangement : 

A 

D  Fj 

G  bTdS 

If  G  E,   g4 

F  A,   CS 

Hence  it  contains  the  decad  of  E^  and  four  additional  notes  Ay  F,  B\},  D,. 
This  furnished  the  power  of  playing  in  G  and  G  major  with  the  grave  seconds  ^1, 
Di,  and  in  F  migor  without  the  grave  second  G^.  Also  in  Ex  major  without  the 
grave  second  F^^.  It  can  modulate  perfectly  from  G  major  to  the  dominant  G 
major,  and  from  G  major  to  its  relative  minor  J^,.  Also  from  J^i  major  to  its 
tonic  minor  E^.  But  it  cannot  modulate  perfectly  from  C  major  to  its  relative 
minor  Ax^  because  of  the  absence  of  Fjj^  occasionally  used  in  the  subdominant.  It 
can  modulate  perfectly  from  G  major  to  its  subdominant  C  major,  and  thence  to 
its  subdominant  F  major,  less  the  grave  second  Gp  A  considerable  variety  of 
harmony  and  modulation  therefore  lies  open  to  it,  but  most  pieces  require  special 
arrangement. 

It  has  been  found  advisable  to  put  Di,  A^  on  the  white  studs  and  D,  A  on  the 
V  black  studs,  that  is  D  on  the  D^  stud,  and  A  on  the  A\}  stud,  and  then  I  put  Dj^ 
on  the  E\}  stud.    In  other  respects  the  fingering  is  unaltered. 

Tufimg.    The  tuners  of  Laohenal's  conoer-  numbers  except  for  D|  ^„  whioh  had  to  be 

tins  factory  (4  Little  James  Street,  Bedford  distinguished  from  D,  A.    In  writing  mosie 

Bow,  London,  W.G.)  were  able  to  tmie  with  for  it  I  generally  assume  the  valnes  of  D  and 

sufficient  oorreotness  from  the  following  direo-  ^,  as  in  the  key  of  C,  and  distingnish  Dj  bj  a 

tions.  downstroke  (as  '6'b  in  the  diagram  p.  22c), 

Make  the  8  major  chords  CE^O,  GB,X>,  and  il  by  an  apstroke  (as  *'/'  in  the  same  dia- 

DF^tA\  FAfi,  B\>D^F\  Afi^^E^,  E^G^B^,  gram),  but  occasionally  to  prevent  amUgnity 

B,Z)^  F^t  perfect  without   beats.    In  giving  I  also  use  the  up  strdLe  to  A  u>d  the  down 

these  directions  I  avoided  using  the  inferior  stroke  to  A^, 

4.  Mb.  Colin  Bbown's  Voice  Habhonium. 

Mr.  Colin  Brown,  Euing  Lecturer  on  the  Science,  Theory,  and  History  of 
Music  in  the  Andersonian  University  at  Glasgow  (see  p.  259^2,  note  ^),  has  invented 
the  following  keyboard,  a  full  sized  model  of  which  is  in  the  Science  Collections  at 
the  South  Kensington  Museum,  Room  Q.    By  the  kindnose.of  Mr.  Brown  I  am 

Digitized  by  V^OOQIC 


SECT.  P. 


EXPERIMENTAL  JUST  INTONATION. 


471 


enabled  to  give  a  perspective  view  of  his  keyboard  in  fig.  67.    He  calls  his  instru- 
xxient  <  The  Voice  Harmonium/ 


PLAN   OF   KBYBOABD. 


1 

i 

ejlt 

2S,« 

at 

gjit 

B.n 



ct 

A«5 

^,t 

CI 

M 

A« 

Ft 

G,« 

B 

M 

c,t 

E 

*".« 

A 

a,« 

c.« 

d^ 

D 

gjt 

^1 

0 

c^ 

E, 

a 

^x 

c 

fa 

6. 

A 

0, 

J5b 

h. 

c, 

Eb 
d. 

Fi 

A\> 

<h 

c. 



D\> 

9t 

B.b 



JB,b 

Ob 

Cb 

A,\> 

Cb 

Fb 

Fio.  67. 


DUODBNABT  ABBANaXMBNT. 


White 
Digitals 

Oolottred 
Digitals 

Peg 
Digitals 

c« 

E,t 

MS 

n 

Alt 

cjf« 

B 

A« 

/^« 

E 

«.l 

M 

A 

c.« 

a 

D 

Fit 

a^ 

Q 

B| 

^ 

C 

E, 

M 

F 

Ai 

c,« 

Bb 

A 

/^ 

Eb 

G, 

ft. 

Ab 

Cx 

«t 

Db 

Fx 

«« 

Ob 

B,b 

d. 

Cb 

E,b 

9t 

The  notation  used  by  Mr.  CJolin 
Brown,  as  shewn  in  fig.  67,  is  dilfe-  m 
rent  from  that  used    above.    The 
colamn  of  Fifths,  so  far  as  it  is  there 
shewn,  is 

'Dby  Ab,  Eb,  Bb,  F,  C,  G.  D, 
A',E\B^Ft. 

The  rest  of  his  notation  need  not  be 
specified,  because  it  does  not  appear 
in  fig.  67. 


By  the  duodenary  arrangement  it  is  seen  that  the  onlv  duodenation  contem- 
plated was  by  Fifths  up  and  down  from  the  duodene  of  Bi\}  to  that  of  DA.  But 
it  was  not  viewed  in  this  light.  It  was  rather  considered  as  a  series  ot  major 
scales  modulating  into  the  dominant  or  subdominant,  and  also  into  the  relative 
minor.    The  tonic  minor  was  always  taken  one  comma  flatter  Uian  the  tonic 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


472  ADDITIONS  BY  THE   TRANSLATOR-  app.  xx. 

major ;  thus  the  tonio  minor  of  C  major  was  considered  to  be  the  relative  minor  of 
E\}  major,  that  is  Ci  minor,  which,  as  shewn  by  Prof.  Hehnholtz's  theories  and  the 
annexed  duodenary  arrangement,  has  not  a  single  tone  in  common  with  C  major.* 
The  third  column  was  considered  merely  as  containing  the  major  Sevenths  and 
major  Sixths  of  the  relative  minor  scale  and  accounted  of  subordinate  interest. 

The  arrangement  of  the  keyboard  is  highlv  ingenious.  Observing  that  in  the 
major  scale  there  are  four  notes  in  the  column  of  Fifths,  and  three  in  the 
column  of  Thirds,  it  became  evident  that  each  note  of  the  first  would  last  during  four 
successive  modulations  into  the  dominant,  whereas  each  of  the  latter  would  last  only 
through  three  modulations.  Hence  the  digitals  containing  the  former  were  made 
four  parts  long,  and  those  containing  the  latter  three  parts  long.  In  going  up  a 
series  of  Fifths  each  digital  advanced  one  part.  In  the  plan  the  longer  digitals  with  ' 
only  one  note  name  were  four  parts  long,  and  were  left  white.  It  wiU  be  seen 
that  F  is  one  part  lower  than  0,  G  than  O,  O  ilian  Z>,  and  so  on.  Then  D  stands 
two  parts  higher  than  C.    E,  a  major  tone  above  D,  stands  also  two  parts  above 

H  D.  The  long  white  digitals,  read  diagonally,  give  therefore  the  first  colunm  in  the 
duodenary  arrangement.  Immediately  below  each  is  a  short  coloured  digital,  dis- 
tinguished in  the  plan  by  having  two  names  of  notes  on  it.  The  lower  is  that  of  the 
note  corresponding  to  the  digital,  and  it  is  exacUy  one  comma  flatter  than  that  of 
the  long  white  digital  above  it.  By  this  means  the  diagonal  series  of  coloured 
digitals  give  the  column  of  Thirds.  Any  white  long  digital  is  separated  from  the 
white  digital  next  below  it  by  a  short  coloured  digital,  and  hence  it  corresponds 
to  a  rise  of  seven  Fifths  from  the  note  of  the  lower  white  digital,  and  this  rise 
gives  the  Pythagorean  sharp,  or  114  cents.  Consequently  the  coloured  digital 
which  separates  two  white  ones,  being  a  comma  of  22  cents  flatter  than  the  upper 
white  one,  is  92  cents  sharper  than  the  lower  one.  This  gives  the  complete 
order,  white  C,  coloured  Cx  f^  above  it,  and  white  G&  above  that ;  coloured  C| 
below  G,  and  white  C\}  below  C^  Then  each  digitfd  on  the  right  begins  two 
parts  higher,  corresponding  to  a  major  Second,  or  two  Fifths  less  an  Octave; 
and  the  fingerboard  is  complete  for  the  first  two  colunms  of  the  duodenary 

^  arrangement.  In  the  fingerboard  itself,  as  shewn  in  fig.  67,  the  lower  and 
upper  digitals  are  out  through  at  the  dotted  line,  but  they  have  been  continued  in 
the  plan  to  shew  the  arrangement.  Beginning  then  with  any  white  digital  as 
G  we  play  the  major  scale  in  a  horizontal  line  passing  through  the  letters  D,  Bi 
on  the  plan,  and  giving  C,  D  (both  white),  JSr,  (coloured),  F,  G  (both  white),  Jj, 
^1  (both  coloured).  The  fingering  is  absolutely  the  same  for  all  major  scales 
whatever  note  is  used  as  the  white  digital  to  commence  with.  The  grave  second 
D,  is  furnished  by  the  coloured  digital  below  the  usual  second. 

For  the  relative  minor,  suppose  the  descending  form  with  three  minor  chords 
is  used ;  another  line  not  quite  horizontal  through  Di,AiBi  gives  AiBi  G  Di  Ei 
F  G  Ai.  To  make  the  dominant  chord  major,  ukL  hence  change  g  into  gjj^^  touch 
the  small  peg  which  rises  out  of  the  left-hand  comer  of  the  Ai  digital,  and  it  gives 
a  diatonic  Semitone  of  112  cents  below  A^,  that  is,  the  leading  note  to  ^|.  This 
peg  is  immediately  to  the  right  of  the  G  digital.  In  the  same  way  to  make  the 
subdominant  chord  major,  and  hence  change  /  into /^,  use  the  peg/^  immedi- 

H  ately  to  the  right  of  the  white  digital  F.  The  names  of  these  pegs  are  written  in 
small  letters  on  the  plan.  We  could  by  introducing  the  Cg  j|l  peg  next  to  the  G 
digital,  play  the  complete  major  scale  of  J[|,  as  Ai  B^  c^fi  J^i  E^  f^%  ^gj]!  il,,  and 
all  major  scales  beginning  with  a  coloured  digital  would  be  fingered  m  the  same 
way.  Thus  we  could  play  the  major  scale  of  A^  and  modulate  into  the  tonic 
minor  scale  of  A^.  Similarly  we  could  play  the  major  scale  of  c^  and  modulate 
into  the  tonic  minor  scale  of  Ci.  But  the  fingering  was  not  intended  for  this, 
and  hence  it  is  not  so  convenient. 

*  Mr.  Brown  considers  (Music  in  Common  upon  the  same  tone  of  absolute  piteh.'    Bai 

Things,  p.  35)   that  C  major  and   C,  minor  on  his  own  fingerboard  we  have  the  major 

have  one  tone  in  common,  F.    This  makes  his  scale  of  A^^  and  what  is,  according  to  ProL 

descending  form  of  the  scale  of  C,  minor  read  Hehnholts,  the  relative  minor  of  C,  both  com- 

upwards  c^  d^  eb  /  Qi  ab  bb  c/,  where  I  should  menoing  with  A^,  the  one  having  the  three 

use  /|,  which  is  ready  to  hand  on  the  instru-  minor  chords  d^fa^^  a,  C6,,6|  fjrdi  and  the  other 

ment,  if  desired.  Mr.  Brown  asserts  (ibid,  p.  35)  the  three  corresponding  major  chords  d^/jl  a.^ 

that  *  it  is  impossible  to  build  a  major  and  a,  CjS  0p  «,  g.it  dj. 


its  tonic  minor  scale  in  trus  key  relationship^ 


Digitized  by 


Google 


SECT.  F, 


EXPERIMENTAL  JUST  INTONATION. 


473 


For  the  sake  of  perfect  uniformity  in  fingering  the  difference  of  a  skhisma 
is  tnned,  or  intended  so  to  be.  Observing  the  two  dotted  lines  in  the  duodenary 
arrangement,  we  see  that  no  note  between  them  differs  from  any  other  by  less 
than  a  comma,  but  the  8  notes  A\},  D\},  G\},  C\},  and  G^,  F^,  B^)},  E^\}  below 
them,  and  6  notes  Di%  Ax%  ^i$  and  f^%%  c^jtff*  9^  above  them,  differ  from 
notes  between  them  by  a  skhisma  only,  the  first  eight  being  respectively  a  skhisma 
flatter  than  0^%  Ci^,  Fx%  Bi  and  b^,  ejf^,  ajj^,  dj^,  and  the  last  six  re- 
spectively a  skhisma  sharper  than  £[>,  ^,  F  and  Gi,  D^,  Aiy  which  notes  all  lie 
between  the  dotted  lines. 

The  tuning  was  effected,  as  the  duodenary  arrangement  shews,  by  Fiflhs  and 
major  Thirds,  and  to  overcome  the  difficulty  of  the  latter  the  combinational  tone 
was  employed* 

5.  Rev.  Henby  Liston's  Obgan. 

The  pitch  of  Mr.  Liston's  notes  has  been  calculated  from  the  data  furnished  by  % 
Mr.  Farey,  Philosophical  Magazvtie,  vol.  xxxix.  p.  418.     Mr.  Liston's  Essay  on 
Perfect  Intonation  was  published  in  1812.      The   following  is  the  duodenary 
arrangement  of  his  notes : — 




__ 

_^ 

c« 

E,n 



_ 

— 

— 

D» 

Ft 

A^Z 

C,«| 



— 

— 

(3f» 

B 

A« 

y,5l 



lp»b 

A*\, 

C 

E 

G.« 

Bji 



— 

jD»b 

F^ 

A 

0.5 

Ejt 



JJ»btl 

0«b 

J5'b 

D 

F,Z 

^.1 

cjin 

— 

C*b 

£*b 

0 

-Bi 

Dtt 

F,tt 

— 

F*b 

A'\> 

C 

E, 

Gjt 

B.n 

— 

B»bb 

D'\> 

F 

A, 

Ojf 

E^ 

— 

— 

G»b 

Bb 

A 

PJl 

A^ 

— 

— 

C»b 

Jg?b 

Gx 

— 

— 

JP»b 

Ab 

c\ 

.^ 



— 

— 

B»bb 

— 



— 

The  two  isolated  tones  on  the  left  were,  I  believe,  added  for  the  sake  of  tuning. 
It  is  evident  that  Mr.  Listen  contemplated  considerable  modulation,  and  provided 
for  the  tonic  as  well  as  relative  minors. 


6.  Oen.  Pebbonbt  Thompson's  Enhabmonio  Organ. 

This  was  the  organ  constructed  by  Robson  for  Gen.  T.  Perronet*  Thompson, 
which  I  took  Prof.  Helmholtz  to  hear,  as  described  in  App.  XVIQ.  p.  423.  It  had 
three  manuals,  each  with  a  complicated  fingerboard,  and  is  completely  described 
and  figured  in  the  General's  Principles  and  Practice  of  Just  Intonationy  which 
is  also  full  of  curious  musical  information.  It  contained,  on  the  whole,  40  tones 
to  the  Octave,  and  had  considerable  power  of  modulation,  as  shewn  by  the  follow- 
ing table.  But  the  gaps  left  indicate  that  the  problem  had  not  been  completely 
grasped* 


Duodenary  Arrangement. 

c 

E 

._ 

- 

_ 

F' 

A 

o,t 





B>b 

D 

i^.« 

^a 



E'b 

G 

•Bi 

DJt 

^at 

A^b 

C 

E, 

0^ 

Ba 

2>'b 

F 

^. 

CJI 

Ea 

G'b 

Bb 

A 

P.t 

^a 

— 

Eb 

0, 

B, 

A« 

— 

Ab 

0, 

E, 

eta 

— 

^. 

^ 

Oji 

7 

9 

9 

8 

7 

f 


Ko.  of  tones 


*  On  the  organ  iieell  the  name  is  painted  as  Peronet  with  one  r,  bat  the  General  printed 
and  wrote  his  name  with  rr. 


Digitized  by  V^OOQIC 


474 


ADDITIONS  BY  THE   TRANSLATOR. 


APP. 


7.  Mr.  Henbt  Ward  Poole's  Organ, 

See  p.  323d,  note  t»  and  App.  XVIII.  p.  423a,  for  references  to  Mr.  Poole's 
keyboards.  His  papers  are  in  Silliman's  Americcm  Journal  of  Arts  and  Sciences, 
1850,  vol.  ix.  pp.  68-83,  199-216;  1867,  ^o^«  ^^-  PP*  i~2>  (which  contains  thm 
diagrams  of  his  keyboard,  here  reproduced  as  figs.  68, 69,  70,  p.  475,  by  the  pholo- 
graphic  processes  of  the  Typo-etching  Company) ;  1868,  vol.  xlv.  p.  289.  The 
first  papers  contain  Mr.  Poole's  theory  and  an  acconnt  of  his  Euharmonic  Organ, 
constructed  by  himself  and  Mr.  Joseph  Abbey,  of  Newburyport,  Massachusetts, 
having  an  ordinary  fingerboard  and  a  pedal  to  change  the  pipes  that  it  affected,  and 
playing  from  the  major  key  of  I)\)  with  5  flats  to  that  of  B  with  c  sharps.  The  12 
digitals  brought  into  action  by  each  pedal  produced  the  7  notes  of  the  major  scale,  the 
leading  note  of  the  relative  minor  scale,  the  perfect  Seventh,  and  three  others 
belonging  to  adjoining  scales,  of  which  only  one  (the  grave  Second)  is  specified. 
This  arrangement,  which  was  actually  used  in  Boston,  was  abandoned,  because  it 
was  found  advisable  to  have  all  the  notes  under  command  of  the  hand  ^th- 
out  pedal  action,  and  to  use  pedals  for  the  bass  only.  But  the  new  keyboard  in 
its  complete  form  does  not  appear  from  Mr.  Poole's  papers  to  have  advanced 
beyond  the  stage  of  a  cardboard  model,  although  more  recent  simplifications, 
with  24  and  48  tones  to  the  Octave,  have  been  practically  worked  out.  To  these 
reference  will  be  made  at  the  conclusion  of  this  notice. 

In  his  theory  of  this  keyboard,  to  which  all  subsequent  remarks  refer,  Mr.  Poole 
recognised  5  series  of  Fifths,  namely  those  in  cols.  5,  6,  7  of  the  Duodenarium, 
p.  463,  and  two  others  interposed  which  may  be  numbered  as  ^5  and  ^6,  because  they 
contun  notes  which  are  a  septimal  conmia  (63  :  64,  or  27  cents)  flatter  than  the 
corresponding  notes  in  cols.  5  and  6.  These  are  given  in  the  following  duodenary 
arrangement  of  his  notes.  But  instead  of  my  superior  and  inferior  numbers  he 
used  varieties  of  type,  as  shewi^  in  the  letterpress  below  fi^g.  70,  which  was  photo- 
graphed from  the  original  at  the  same  time  as  the  fig.  itself. 


IF 


Dttodena/ry  Arrangement  of  Mr.  Poolers  100  Tones. 


Cols. 


No.  of  tones 


Thus  ool.  5,  or  *  key  notes,*  was  represented 
by  Roman  capitals,  as  G,  D,  and  had  white 
digitals ;  col.  6,  or  *  Thirds,*  by  Roman  small 
letters,  as  b,  e  (this  was,  in  fact,  Hauptmann's 


5 

'5 

6 

'6 

7 

Et 

522 

_ 

_ 

_ 



AZ 

1020 

- 

c,ti 

206 

— 

- 

— 

m 

318 
816 

- 

F,nt 

704 

— 

- 

A^tlO^Q 

01 

»Gf«" 

789 

B,n 

2 

— 

- 

D^t  388 

c% 

114 

»C« 

87 

E,Z 

s<» 

'E,t 

473 

Qjil  886 

n 

612 

'n 

58s 

M 

998 

M.ff 

971 

C^J   184 

B 

mo 

»J3 

1083 

A« 

296 

'A« 

269 

F^Z  682 

E 

408 

'E 

381 

o,t 

794 

'Gi« 

767 

B^    1 180 

A 

906 

'A 

879 

c,« 

92 

'0,5 

65 

E^     478 

D 

204 

'D 

177 

^,« 

590 

'F,t 

563 

A^      976 

Q 

702 

'Q 

675 

-Bi 

1088 

'B, 

io6i 

I>J      274 

C 

0 

'C 

"73 

E, 

386 

'Ey 

359 

Ott    772 

F 

498 

'F 

471 

^, 

884 

'A, 

857 

c^     70 

Bb 

996 

'J5b 

969 

^. 

182 

'A 

155 

F^      568 

E\> 

294 

'Eb 

267 

Gi 

680 

'G. 

653 

B,      1066 

A\> 

792 

'A\> 

„ 

c, 

1 178 

'C, 

"51 

Er       364 

D\> 

90 

Wb 

63 

Fr 

476 

'F, 

449 

A^       862 

Q\> 

588 

'Gb 

561 

B,b 

974 

'B,b 

947 

D,        160 

C\> 

1086 

'Cb 

I0S9 

E,b 

272 

%b 

245 

Q,       658 

F\> 

882 

'Fb 

357 

A^b 

770 

M,b 

743 

C,      I 156 

JBbb 

'Bbb 

85s 

D,b 

68 

'Ab 

41 

— 

E\>\^ 

150 

'Ebb 

153 

0,b 

566 

'G.b 

539 

— 

'Abb 

652 

— 

'C,b 

1037 

— 

22 

20 

21 

19 

18 

original  plan  in  1853,  snpr^  p.  2766),  and  had 
black  digitals  rising  0*4  inch ;  ooL  7,  or  *  domi- 
nant Thirds,  minor,*  by  italic  small  letten, 
as  d%  ,  and  had  flat  blue  digitals  rising  0*1  inch 


Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


B^CT.  F.  EXPERIMENTAL  JUST  INTONATION. 

Pio.  68. 


475 


PBBSPBOnVB  VIBW  Of  MB.  P00I.|'B  KBTBOABD. 

7ta.  69, 


m 


Fio.  70. 


1 

h 

Sot* 

Ejl, 

Do 


2b 


5  s|^ 

•0 

V4 

8m 

n 

p-Zk    S«  1  ri  1  Do 

IB             mir         T             viwiw 

•  tf          filQ     U.U    S:«           ff:10         8:«     if:U 
SOVBLB  »IA«Oai«  •O&I.B. 

T       m            w   I          ■            m         IT    T 

t:ie     U<U    S:« 


PLAN  OF  MB.   POOLB'S  KBTBOABD. 


SECTION  THBOUOH  AB  FIO.  69. 


above  the  white  keys,  marked  with  heraldic 
horizontal  cross  lines  in  fig.  68;  col.  *5,  or 
'  Sevenths,*  by  antique  Boman  capitals,  with 
the  index ',  as  p,  and  had  red  digitals,  marked 
with  heraldic  vertical  cross  lines  in  fig.  68,  and 
rising  only  0*05  inch  above  the  white  digitals ; 
col.  '6,  or  *  dominant  Sevenths,  minor,'  had 
antique  small  letters  and  an  index  as  d',  and 
ycXUm  keys  marked  with  heraldic  dots  in 
fig.  68,  rising  0*15  inch  above  the  white  keys. 
The  length  of  a  white  digital  for  col.  5  being 
taken  as  4  inches,  that  of  the  black  digital  for 
coL  6  was  3  inches,  and  that  of  each  of  the 
coloured  keys  i  inch.  Fig.  68  gives  a  per- 
spective view  of  this  arrangement,  with  Mr. 
Poole's  names  of  the  notes  on  the  digitals  for 
the  key  of  C  major  and  some  adjacent  notes. 
Fig.  69  gives  a  plan  of  this  arrangement  with 
solfeggio  names,  except  for  two  notes  (pro- 
nounced with  ItaJian  vowels),  to  shew  that  it 
serves  for  any  key  beginning  with  a  white 
digital  and  a  cross  line  A  B  *  through  the 


centre  of  the  third  quarter  (inch)  of  the  key- 
note.' Fig.  70  gives  the  solfeggio  names  of  the 
notes  thus  cut,  with  perspective  views  above  of 
the  remaining  parts  of  the  digitals  furthest 
from  the  player.  Underneath  the  solfeggio 
names  are  the  relative  number  of  vibrations  ^ 
of  each  note,  taking  do  as  48.  Below  this 
again  are  the  numbers  of  the  notes  in  Mr, 
Poole's  *  triple  diatonic*  or  trichordal  scale, 
with  the  ratios  of  their  intervals,  and  also  the 
numbers  of  the  notes  in  his  *  double  diatonic  ' 
or  dichordal  scale  (see  p.  344c,  note  *).  And 
finally  the  three  last  lines  give  the  names  of 
the  notes  as  he  writes  them,  supposing  the 
first  white  digitals  to  give  O  (key  of  iff  or  one 
sharp),  C  (natural  key,  where  the  mysterious 
symbol  may  be  meant  for  an  n— that  is, 
*  natural,'  but  seems  to  have  been  reversed  by 
the  wood-engraver),  and  F  (key  of  i  b  or  one 
fiat).  Interpreted  into  our  symbols,  with  the 
interval  in  cents  from  the  lowest  note  in  each 
line,  these  will  be : 


Digitized  by 


Google 


476 


ADDITIONS  BY   THE   TRANSLATOR. 


APP.  XX- 


rel.  vib. 

Kotes 

in 
fig.  70 

cents 

Solfeggio 

colours 


48 

54 
a 

52i 

60 
ft. 

64 

72 
d' 

75 
d,'n 

80 

84 

90 

96 

9 

'«! 

c' 

^\ 

'/' 

'i'. 

iT 

c 

d 

'di 

X 

/ 

g 

9i^ 

«i 

'6b 

c' 

F 
0 

G 

'0, 

Bb 

c 

c^ 

rf. 

'eb 

<H 

/ 

204 

155 

386 

498 

702 

772 

884 

969 

1088 

laoo 

Do 

Be 

Mi 

Fa 

Sol 

La 

Se 

Si 

Do 

white 

white 

yellow 

black 

white 

white 

blue 

black 

red 

black 

white 

If  we  took  only  the  black  and  white  digitals, 
the  arrangement  of  the  keyboard  would  be 
like  Mr.  Colin  Brown's ;  but  this  was  an  acci- 
dent, Mr.  Brown  having  never  seen  the  draw- 
ings of  Mr.  Poole's  keyboard.    Both  arrange- 

%  nients  arose  from  the  column  of  Fifths  in  the 
Decad  (p.  4596)  containing  four,  and  the  column 
of  Thirds  oiUy  three  notes. 

The  great  peculiarity  of  Mr.  Poole's  board, 
where  Mr.  Brown  differs  from  it  entirely,  is  in 
the  introduction  of  the  columns  '5  and  6, 
both  containing  natural  Sevenths,  and  tiieir 
amalgamation,  as  it  were,  with  the  col.  7 
(which  Mr.  Brown  alone  uses)  in  three  short 
flat  digitals,  placed  beside  the  black,  and  hence 
of  the  same  length.  They  are  placed  from 
front  to  back,  as  red,  yellow,  blue— that  is,  as 
two  Sevenths  and  a  Third,  which  belong  to 
three  different  keys.  Thus  in  fig.  68,  to  the 
left  of  the  black  digital  for  E^  (marked  e),  lie 
three  coloured  digitals,  i)  the  red  'jE7b  (of  which 
the  name  is  not  marked  in  the  fig.),  the  natural 
Seventh  of  the  tonic  in  the  key  olF\  2)  the 
yellow  'Di  (marked  d'),  the  natural  Seventh  of 

^  the  dominant  of  the  relative  minor  in  the  key 


of  C,  which  is  e^  gfi  6,  '(2, ;  3)  the  red  D^ 
(marked  (2]t ),  the  leading  note  (or  major  Third 
of  the  dominant  &,  d^  /,  'a,)  of  the  relative 
minor  in  the  key  of  Q,  The  situation  of  th^e 
digitals  is  such  that  the  lowest  (or  red)  digital 
gives  the  natural  seventh  of  the  white  digital 
immediately  adjoining  (below  in  the  figure, 
compare  C  :  'C,  F  :  'F).  The  middle  (or 
yellow)  digital  gives  the  natural  Seventh  of 
the  black  digital,  the  right-hand  top  comer  of 
which  touches  its  left-hand  bottom  caroa 
(compare  D^  :  *i),,  printed  d  d^).  The  upper- 
most (or  blue)  digital  gives  a  note  which  is  a 
small  Semitone  (24  :  25  =  70  cents)  sharper 
than  the  note  of  the  white  digital  on  the  left 
(compare  D  :  Z).J  ,  marked  D  :  ds),  and  a 
diatonic  Semitone  (15  :  16  or  112  cents)  flatter 
than  the  note  of  the  black  digital  on  the  ri^ 
(compare  D^S  :  JEr„  marked  d%  :  e).  The  only 
oigital  placed  out  of  ascending  order  from  left 
to  right  is  the  yellow  one,  which  should  have 
come  between  the  two  white  digitids  for  so 
and  re,  but  has  been  displaced  from  motives 
of  convenience. 


Mr.  Poole,  as  Mr.  Colin  Brown  afterwards,  provided  only  for  modulations  from 
major  keys  into  the  dominant  major,  subdominant  major,  and  relative  mimn'. 
For  the  modulation  into  the  tonic  minor,  therefore,  he  had  to  flatten  by  a  comma. 
Thus  C  minor  was  considered  to  be  the  relative  minor  of  E[}  major  instead  of  E^\^ 
major.  And  although,  in  deference  to  Mr.  Listen  and  Gen.  Perronet  Thompson, 
he  also  made  a  provision  for  temporarily  introducing  the  tonic  minor  if  desired, 
giving  col.  4  of  the  Duodenarium  {Silliman,  vol.  xhv.  p.  18,  art.  35),  he  did  not 
require  it  himself.  '  In  the  theory  I  have  advocated,'  he  says,  '  the  major  keys  are 
based  on  the  first  series  of  sounds,'  p.  474c,  col.  5,  '  and  the  minor  keys  on  the 
Sixths  of  the  major  keys,'  ibid.  col.  6.  '  That  there  must  be  such  a  relation  and 
order  is  inevitable.'  But  this  does  not  exclude  taking  minor  keys  also  upon  notes 
of  col.  5  as  a  base,  considered,  if  desired,  as  the  Sixths  of  major  keys  on  the  not^ 
of  col.  4  of  the  Duodenarium.  Otherwise  tonality  is  destroyed  by  constant  shifts 
of  a  comma  severely  felt  on  justlv  intoned  instruments. 

[  Mr.  Poole  was  £Jso  aware  of  the  alteration  by  a  skhisma,  and  of  the  consequent 
reduction  of  the  number  of  pipes.  He  also  refers  to  the  53  division,  but  he  does 
not  seem  to  adopt  either,  and  is  not  distinct  enough  on  these  points  for  me  to 
state  his  conclusions  with  certainty.  In  the  duodenary  arrangement,  I  have  by 
dotted  lines  marked  the  places  where  tlie  skhisma  comes  into  play,  and  by  affixing 
tiie  cents  to  each  note  have  shewn  how  it  acts. 

It  will  be  seen  that  Mr.  Poole  had  100  notes  to  the  octave,  of  which  39  arose 
from  the  harmonic  Sevenths.  If  the  skhisma  were  neglected  there  would  remain 
only  36  tertian  and  20  septimal,  or  in  all  56  tones  to  the  octave.  The  duodenary 
arrangement  has  been  taken  from  Mr.  Poole's  Enharmonic  table  (SiUinian^  xliv. 
p.  13),  consisting  of  19  lines  similar  to  the  3  at  the  bottom  of  fig.  70.  He  adds 
the  following  example  of  the  fingering  of  chords  upon  his  keyboard,  the  double 
numbers  indicating  '  that  the  key  is  touched  with  one  finger  and  immediatelj 
changed  for  another.*  The  duodenals  and  mark  of  the  natural  Seventh  are  accord- 
ing to  my  notation,  sect.  E.  art.  26,  p.  465c.  The  upper  figures  refer  to  the  Notes 
which  follow. 

Digitized  by  V^OOQIC 


«ECT.  F. 


EXPERIMENTAL  JUST  INTONATION. 


477 


C. 


«3 


A,. 


— . ■ ■   ■         ^  M ^^ — — 


"2sr 
3 

+ 


^*         3 


3 

I 
+ 


4 

2 
+ 


3 

I 
+ 


3 

I 
+ 


3 

+  1 

+ 


4 

3* 
1  + 


3 
I 

+ 


4 

I 
+ 


34 

I 
+ 


i 


3z^g>.gg^-g:^.^  I  ^~^ 


?i: 


S 


321 


m 


=^=f3^ 


4        3 
I         I 

+        + 


4 

I 
+ 


34 

I 
+ 


3 

I 
+ 


2      3      4 

I  2 

+  + 


3 

I 
+ 


*  ^o^. — I.  Sabdominant  chord/  and  a/. 

*  2.  Dominant  with  Seventh  /. 

*  3.  Same  with  Ninth  a!  [not  a/,  and  hence 
causing  a  dnodenation  into  the  dominant  (?, 
but  forming  the  second  chord  in  Poolers  di- 
chordal  scale  of  C.  Of  course,  ''f  itself  is  not 
in  the  duodene  of  G,  but  when  these  natural 
hievenths  are  introduced  the  special  marks  are 
used.    See  supri,  p.  349a]. 

*  4.  Dominant  Seventh. 

'  5.  Dominant  of  the  relative  minor,  the 
iBeventh  'd,  may  be  added  [it  is  added  here, 
but  to  secure  the  intonation  a  duodenation 
into  the  relative  is  marked]. 

'6.  Subdominant  with  Seventh  [duodena- 
tion into  F,  therefore]. 

'  7.  Grave  second  or  Sixth  of  subdomi- 
nant [as  Uie  duodenal  gives  the  root  F^  the 
<2/'  is  sufficiently  marked]. 

*8.  The  flattened  note  should  be  made 
natural  in  the  next  chord*  [meaning  *next 
chord  but  one,*  namely  at  8,  so  that  there  is 
again  a  duodenation  from  i^  to  C  as  marked]. 

The  above  examples  will  shew  how  Mr. 
Poole  treats  the  chord  of  the  dominant  Seventh 
and  the  major  Ninth.  The  three  last  chords 
are  added  to  shew  his  treatment  of  the  chord 
of  the  diminished  Seventh  (SiZZtman,  vol.  ix. 
pp.  78-80).  He  considers  the  first  of  these 
chords  to  be  merely  the  chord  of  Q  with  the 
dominant  Seventh,  </  6/  d"  '/',  which  is  of 
course  in  the  major  scale  of  G,  but  this  lies 


within  the  duodene  of  ^p  as  I  have  marked  it,  ir 
including  the  ^F  within  that  duodene,  as  shewn 
in  the  daodenary  arrangement  p.  4740.  Then 
he  supposes  that  in  order  to  resolve  the  chord 
a/  c"  e/'  (the  last  chord),  the  ^  is  altered  in 
the  second  chord  by  '  a  chromatic  Semitone  * 
(that  is,  the  small  Semitone  24  :  25  »  70  cents), 
to  git ,  which  is  necessarily  in  the  duodene  of 
JS7„  bat  this  gi  %  serves  merely  as  '  a  passing 
note  *  to  the  following  a\  and  therefore,  he 
says,  *must  be  thrown  out  when  we  reckon 
the  harmony.*  But  this  will  not  explain  the 
present  use  of  this  chord,  which  is  now  intro- 
duced without  preparation,  and  as  a  means  of 
modulation.  The  ratios  of  the  chord  Mr. 
Poole  gives  are  25  :  30  :  36  :  42,  or  taking 
the  ^,'8  an  Octave  higher,  to  compare  with 
my  form,  it  becomes  30  :  36  :  42  :  50,  that  is 
10  :  12  :  14  :  i6§,  or  in  cents  o,  316,  583,  884, 
Mr.  J.  Paul  White  (see  below  No.  9)  makes  the  % 
ratios  of  the  chord  30  :  35  :  42  :  50,  that  is 
10  :  II*  :  14  :  i6f,  or  in  cents  o,  267,  583, 884. 
The  individual  intervals  in  the  first  are  316, 
267,  301,  and  in  the  second  267,  316,  301',  so 
that  the  two  first  intervals  are  transposed. 
But  in  both  the  interval  of  the  extreme  notes 
is  3  :  5  =  884  cents,  so  that  in  neither  have  we 
a  chord  of  a  diminished  Seventh  at  all,  which 
must  have  919  or  926  cents.  It  is  only  equal 
temperament  which  confuses  the  major  Sixth 
and  diminished  Seventh  together  by  using  900 
cents  for  either  of  them. 


With  regard  to  the  double  diatonic  or  dichordal  scale,  which  Mr.  Poole  always 
Bolfas  as  fah  sol  la  se  do  re  mi  fah  (where  ae  is  the  harmonic  Seventh  to  do), 
80  that  do  is  the  dominant,  he  says  that  '  the  most  beautifol,  varied,  and  ornate 
compositions  are  made  from  the  elements  it  contains.  It  has  the  capacity  in  oer-  «r 
tain  styles  of  music  of  using  with  much  grace  accidentals,  or  chromatics  as  they 
are  called ;  for  example,  the  si,  the  regular  leading  note  to  do,  and  the  soljt,  a 
diatonic  Semitone  to  below  la,  or  the  leading  note  to  the  relative  minor ;  these 
chromatics  always  ascending  a  diatonic  Senutone  (15  :  16)  to  the  notes  above.' 
In  an  example  given  he  also  admits  se  to  be  raised  by  27  cents,  that  is  to  be  the 
regular  Fourth  of  the  triple  or  trichordal  scale,  and  also  allows  the  introduction  of  the 
Sixth  of  this  scale.  Hence  if  we  use  the  duodenary  form  and  represent  the  dichordal 
scale  of  F  by  capitals  and  these  permissive  additions  by  small  letters  we  shall  have 

the  scheme  in  the  margin.  This  gives  the  trichordal 
scale  of  C  major  complete  with  its  grave  second,  and 
gfjjj  also  one  form  of  its  relative  il,  minor  complete,  but 
both  without  the  harmonic  Seventh  of  the  dominants, 
which  of  course  he  would  be  ready  to  add  when  the 
harmony  in  his  view  required  it.  There  is  also  the  complete  trichordal  scale  of  F 
major  without  the  grave  second.  Hence  his  dichordal  scale  resolves  itself  into  a 
means  of  bringing  these  three  scales  into  close  connection,  chiefly  by  help  of  the 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQ IC 


D 

G 

ii 

G 

JS?. 

F 

^1 

bt> 

^B\}    d, 

478 


ADDITIONS  BY  THE  TRANSLATOR. 


APP.  XX- 


chord  of  the  Ninth  CE^G  ^J?|>Z>  in  the  above  scheme.  The  example  that  he  gives 
of  its  use  is  the  accompaniment  to  Figaro's  Numero  quindici  from  Rossini's 
Barbieret  afterwards  smig  as  the  air  Ah  I  che  d'amore  by  Ahnaviva.  This  is  written 
in  G  major.  He  gives  the  scale  thus»  using  my  notation  and  indicating  accidentals 
by  small  letters : 


Double  Diatonic  Scale  in  G,  with  Accidentals. 
G    A    ajjf    Bx    ^C,    c   cj    D    JE?    JP,# 


''t-ZT~T-^r-^^ 


The  Dnodenals  are  mine,  but  as  the  Ninth 
is  not  in  a  single  duodena,  it  can  be  marked 
only  by  giving  the  duodene  containing  all  but 
the  natural  Seventh  and  indicating  that  by  a 
sloping  line  in  the  usual  way.  The  notes  in 
inverted  commas  are  from  Mr.  Poole,  except 
the  bracketed  portions,  which  are  mine. 

*  I.  This  may  be  «,"  [in  that  case  the  three 
first  notes  are  in  the  duodene  of  G]. 

*  2  and  3.  These  may  be  c"  as  well  as  V' 
[in  the  latter  case  the  whole  run  would  be  in 
D,  the  c"  being  marked  as  V ;  in  the  former, 
the  run  would  be  in  G]. 

*  4.  This  may  be  e,"  [this  will  be  only  if  3 
«  is  c",  so  that  the  whole  run  is  in  (?J. 

*  5.  This  note  is  clearly  and  necessarily  0". 
[In  this  case  3  certainly  should  be  V  and  4 
should  also  be  e",  but  Mr.  Poole  does  not  ob- 
ject to  gf'  a'  6/  c"  d"  e,"  followed  by  e",  saying] 
the  enharmonic  change  from  e"  to  ^',  a  rise  of 
a  conmia,  is  often  required,  and  I  have  proved 
that  it  can  readily  be  made,  for  my  singers, 
who  know  this  change  of  a  comma  as  well  as 
others  know  the  Tone  or  Semitone,  will  give 
it,  even  without  accompaniment,  with  perfect 
accuracy,  as  proved  by  the  harmony  after- 
wards supplied  as  a  test.    All  this  variety 

Mr.  Poole  has  also  devised  an  enharmonic  keyboard  pedal  for  the  bass  of  his 
organ,  but  then  confines  himself  entirely  to  cols.  5»  ^5,  6.  The  'keynotes '  cor- 
responding to  the  white  digitals  are  in  front  in 
order  of  f^hs  from  left  to  right.  Behind  them, 
^  at  a  higher  elevation,  are  the  major  Thirds  lying 
between  them.  The  Sevenths  are  in  a  back  row 
behind  their  Fifths.    This  is  indicated  by  the  letters  in  the  margin. 

Mr.  H.  W.  Poole  is  a  native  of  Salem  (afterwards  Danvers,  now  Peabody), 
Massachusetts,  U.S.,  and  is  now  Professor  of  Public  Instruction  in  the  Oovem- 
ment  Institute  of  the  city  of  Mexico,  whence  he  kindly  wrote  to  me  on  9  March 
1885,  describing  one  of  his  new  keyboards.  From  this  I  take  the  following 
summary  and  extracts : — 


within  the  limits  of  musical  laws — which  only 
forbid  what  is  disorderly,  complicated,  or  what 
the  ear  will  not  distinguish — adds  to  the  plea- 
sure of  vocal  music,  and  it  is  the  exact  render- 
ing of  all  the  melodies  and  harmonies  which 
gives  the  charm  to  a  good  singer.  [A  little 
difficulty  arises  as  to  tonality.]  When  acutely 
perceptive  of  such  accuracy,  I  had  the  good 
fortune  to  listen  to  Alboni  on  all  the  occasions 
when  it  was  possible  to  do  so.  I  thought  her 
then,  am  still  of  opinion,  that  she  was  the 
best  singer  I  have  ever  heard.  It  is  certain 
that  she  had  a  wonderful  exactness  in  execating 
whatever  she  undertook.  There  was  no  '*  tem- 
perament "  in  her  scales,  and  what  the  strictest 
theory  requires  in  intonation  she  understood 
and  gave.  She  sang  music  whose  analysis 
would  alarm  a  student  with  its  apparent  £ffi- 
culties ;  but  the  delighted  auditors  perceived 
only  a  delicious  and  "easy  "  flow  of  melody.' 
[This  has  been  quoted  at  length  to  add  to  the 
examples  supr&,  p.  325a.] 

6.  Mr.  Poole  says  nothing  about  this  (f*^ 
but  I  presume  he  would  take  it  regularly  as 
v.  In  that  case  the  whole  of  this  would  be 
in  D  major. 


red 

black 

white 


7^b 


^1 


^-Bt> 


^1 


G 


*  I  send  you  a  stereograph  of  a  simple  form 
of  one  of  them  [his  new  manuals] ,  which  v\ 
easier  to  comprehend  than  a  larger  one  with 
48  levers  to  the  octave.  This  may  be  called  a 
working  model,  and  suffices  for  an  organ  or 
pianoforte  for  instruction  or  study  in  effect 
of  chords  and  fingering.  It  is  solidly  con- 
structed in  wood,  ebony,  and  ivory,  and  works 
as  fre<»ly  as  a  common  one.    These  24  levers 


are  a  quarter  of  an  inch  wide,  and  can  pJay 
a  pianoforte  with  hammers  half  the  common 
width,  with  single  strings,  but  larger  and  lightly 
strained  so  as  to  yield  the  wifiTimTiTn  of  tone, 
tension  nearly  to  breaking-point  giving  bad 
tone.'  The  &ager-key8  for  each  Fifth  rise  ^ 
inch,  BO  0  is  ^  inch  above  Ab>  The  white 
digitals  have  the  same  shape  as  in  fig.  69,  but 
from  each  projects  a  narrow  black  finger-kry. 


Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


IBCT.  P.     MR.  BOBANQUET'S  GENERALISED  FINGERBOARD.  479 

Krith   a   note  one  comma  flatter,  giving  the  (x  x  ^  inch  each)  was  allotted.'    They  fill  up 

najor  Thirds,  and  fitting  into  the  left-hand  tne  space  on  the  left-hand  side  of  the  black 

lick  of  the  next  lower  white  digital.    What-  Thirds,  and  are  of  the  shape  of  the  white  digi- 

»ver  white  digital  the  player  begins  with,  the  tals  in  fig.  69,  only  very  much  narrower,  half 

ingeiing  is  the  same,  and  for  major  scales  the  thin    part  being  separated,  and  for  ex- 

mach  Kke  that  for  the  key  of  A  on  the  usual  ample   given   to   A.Jj^ ,  leading   note   to    B^, 

manual.    For  the  Seventh  and  Ninth  of  the  while  the  rest,  including  the  wide  part,  is 

Dichordal  system  separate  digitals  must  be  given  to  '^b,  harmonic  Seventh  to  Bb.    *  My 

kouched.     Mr.  Poole  can  arrange  for  a  minor  keyboard  admits  of  eiqual  facility  in  execution 

on  the  same  tonic,  but  thinks  it  an  extrava-  and  in  taking  the  chords,  with  the  common 

ganoe.     *  The  diatonic  scales  with  the  broad  one  of  12.    I  think  its  first  utility  will  be  for 

ivory  keys  (larger  than  on  the  common  board)  teaching  singing,  accompanying  violin  players 

are  of  first  importance  ;  next  the  raised  ebony  and  students  of  harmony.    For  this  I  recom- 

digitals  for  Thirds.    The  Sevenths  are  well  mend  the  simple  form  with  less    outlay  of 

provided  for   and  convenient.     The   leading  money.'    This  form  of  Mr.  Poole's  keyboard 

notes  to  major  Thirds  are  introduced  as  dia-  is  therefore  equivalent  to  Mr.  Colin  Brown's 

tonio  Semitones  below  these  black  notes,  and  (No.  4)   with  the    addition    of    the    natural 

eerve  them  as  the  black  ones  do  the  white.  Seventh.                                                               V 
On  my  model  an  equal  space  of  two  measures 

8.  Mb.  Bosanquet'b  Generalised  Fingbbboabd  and  Harmonium. 

Mr.  R.  H.  M.  BoBanquet's  harmomuxn  is  partly  described  in  the  text,  p.  ^-iSc, 
and  its  keyboard  is  figured  and  briefly  explained  in  App.  XIX.,  p.  429.  In  App.  XX. 
sect.  A.  art.  27,  the  nature  of  Mr.  Bosanquet's  cycle  of  53  and  his  notation,  and 
the  value  of  every  one  of  his  notes  are  explained.  In  App.  XX.  sect.  E.  art.  18, 
there  is  an  elaborate  comparison  of  this  cycle  with  just  intonation  giving  the 
number  and  pitch  of  every  note,  and,  ibid,  art.  25,  it  is  shewn  how  such  a  cycle 
might  have  been  suggested  by  just  intonation.  In  sect.  G.  arts.  16  and  17,  the 
methods  of  tuning  the  cycle  of  53  adopted  by  Mr.  Bosanquet  and  Mr.  White  are 
described.  In  the  South  Kensington  Museum,  Science  Collections,  Room  Q,  the 
harmonium  itself  may  be  inspected,  Mr.  Bosanquet  having  presented  it  to  the 
Museum,  as  he  generally  employs  for  his  own  use  an  organ  with  the  same  finger- 
board, and  two  sets  of  pipes,  one  set  for  48  notes  of  the  temperament  advocated 
by  Prof.  Helmholtz  (p.  432a),  with  perfect  major  Thirds  and  Fifths  imperceptibly  ^ 
flattened  by  ^  Skhisma,  answering  to  the  notes  written  with  capital  letters  on  the 
digitals  in  the  following  plan  ;  and  the  other  set  for  36  notes  of  the  meantone  tem- 
perament, brought  into  separate  action  by  a  stop.  The  pipes  are  stopped,  with  a 
screw  plug,  so  that  they  are  more  readily  tuned. 

It  remains  in  this  place  to  give  the  plan  of  the  fingerboard,  shewing  the  dis- 
position of  the  notes  upon  it  both  for  the  53  division  and  meantone  temperament, 
and  to  describe  its  arrangement,  referring  especially  to  App.  XIX.,  p.  429,  fig.  66. 
In  the  present  plan  of  the  keyboard,  all  the  digitals  are  represented  as  of  the 
same  length,  corresponding  to  that  from  tip  to  tip.  This  is  3  inches  in  the  original 
and  is  here  only  i  inch.  At  each  side  runs  a  column  of  figures  i  to  12  con- 
tinually repeated.  It  will  be  observed  that  in  the  first  column  the  lines  terminating 
the  oblongs  come  against  2,  and  that  2  is  at  the  head  pf  the  column.  In  this  case 
the  end  of  each  oblong  gives  a  form  of  c,  and  in  passing  from  one  form  to  another, 
as  c  to  c^  we  have  gained  a  Pythagorean  comma,  which  results  from  taking  12 
Fifths  reduced  to  the  same  Octave.  In  the  g  column  headed;  3  the  lines  are  ^ 
opposite  3  ;  in  the  d  column,  headed  4,  opposite  4 ;  and  so  on ;  each  Fifth  corre- 
sponding to  a  rise  of  ]^  inch  from  tip  to  tip  of  the  digitals,  and  to  a  vertical  rise 
of  iV  i^<)h  &om  level  to  level.  Hence  in  going  from  one  degree  to  another,  as  d 
to  c*  or  4  to  5,  we  go  backwards  12x^  =  3  inches,  and  rise  12  x  1^=  i  inch. 
Mr.  Bosanquet  says  {Mus.  Int.  and  Temp.  p.  20) : — 

'  The  most  important  practical  point  about  the  keyboard  arises  from  its  sym- 
metry ;  that  is  to  say,  from  the  faict  that  every  key  is  surrounded  by  the  same 
definite  arrangement  of  keys,  and  that  a  pair  of  keys  in  a  given  relative  position 
corresponds  always  to  the  same  interval.  From  this  it  follows  that  any  passage, 
chord,  or  combination  of  any  kind,  has  exactly  the  same  form  under  the  fingers 
in  whatever  key  it  is  played.  And  more  than  this,  a  common  chord,  for  instance, 
has  always  the  same  form,  no  matter  what  view  be  taken  of  its  key  relationship. 
Some  simplification  of  this  kind  is  a  necessity  if  these  complex  phenomena  are  to 
be  brought  within  the  reach  of  persons  of  average  ability ;  and  witib  this  particular 
simplification,  the  child  or  beginner  finds  the  work  reduced  to  the  acquirement  of 
one  thing,  where  twelve  have  to  be  learned  on  the  ordinary  keyboard/ 

Digitized  by  VjOOQIC 


480 


ADDITIONS  BY  THE  TEANSLATOB. 


APP.  XI. 


IT 


2 


PLAN   OP  MR.   BOSANQUET'S  GENERALISED   KEYBOARD. 
9         4        II         6  I         8         3        10       5        12        7 

BLACK 


3        10 

BLACK 


c> 


10 


IIS9 

C 

♦4 
e 


3 


dbb 
41 

2 


35 


D'b 
*9 

76 


8 

cff_ 

"db 
117 


7 


BLACK 


^bb 


08  S 
152 


*i3 
d 


d 

193 

A 

12 

d, 


ebb 
234 

II 


88 
228 
^'b 

♦18 

d'U 

"di" 
269 


E\} 

dn 


eb 
310 


16 

d.8^ 


A8 

15 

<i4 


2?^b 
23 

d8  8 
345 


22 


0 
386 


^1 
♦21 

fb 
427 


D,85 
20 


d8 
88 
421 

2^1 
27 


08 
462 


»26 

/ 


f 

503 

J^,8 
=  ^i 

25 

/. 


^38 

24 

/t_ 

WHITB 


G'b 

-2^'8 
32 
/•8 


08  8 
538 


G^b 

-JP8 

31 

ft 


«G' 
36 
9' 


f8  8 
656 


f8 
579 

-^18 

♦30  i 

_/8_' 

~gb 
621 


♦35 
9 


697 


29 


34 

9i 


abb 
738 


F,8  8 
33 

9-2 


A*b 

-G'8 

41 

Sr'8 


f8 
88 
732 

ii»b 
=  G8 
♦40 
g'8 
ff8 
773 

Gf,8 
39 

y8 

ab 
814 


0,Z 

38 

^i8 


B'bb 
45 


98  8 
849 


a 
890 


^1 
•43 


bbb 
931 

42 
a. 


BLACK 


925 

S'b 

♦49 

a»8 


a8 
966 


Bb 

48 
ja8 

bb 

1007 

=B,b 

47 

«.8 


^.8 

=Bjb 

46 


C7«b 
-B» 

I 
6« 


a8  8 
1042 

Cb 

53 
b 


b 
1083 

•52 
2»i 


Ob 
1 124 

51 


C» 


b8 
1 159 

C 

«B,I 
*4 


B.8 
3 


dbb 
41 

B^ 

2 


7 
to 

12 

16 

21 

2S 

30 

34 

38 

43 

47 

52 

3 

7 

to 

to 

to 

to 

to 

to 

to 

to 

to 

to 

to 

to 

I 

6 

10 

15 

19 

24 

28 

32 

37 

41 

46 

50 

1 

The  numbers  in  the  oblongs  and  in  the  lines  at  the  bottom  of  the  Table  are  the  nnmben 
assiimed  to  the  tones  of  Bosanquet's  cycle  of  53,  in  sect.  A.  art.  27,  and  sect  E.  art  18,  and 
shew  their  distribution  on  this  keyboard,  which  was  invented  for  playing  them.  The  smaU 
italic  letters  under  the  numbers  at  the  bottom  of  each  oblong  are  the  transcriptions  of  one  of 
Mr  Bosanquet*s  names,  of  which  all  are  given  in  sect.  A.  ait.  27,  against  the  number  ci 
the  note.  The  stars  preceding  the  numbers  shew  those  which  constitute  the  duodene  of  C. 
The  notes  of  all  other  duodenes  stand  in  the  same  relative  position  to  their  root-  The  capital 
letters  are  48  out  of  the  56  tones  between  the  dotted  and  thick  lines  in  the  Duodenarinm, 
sect  E  art.  18,  and  those  following  the  sign  =  are  other  tones  diflfering  from  them  by  a 
skhisma,  which  are  purposely  identified  with  them,  see  sect.  E.  art.  25.  These  are  the  48  tones 
used  by  Mr  Bosanquet  for  his  organ,  but  he  names  them  as  at  the  bottom  of  each  digital.  The 
small  thick  Roman  letters  at  the  top  of  each  oblong  are  36  tones  of  the  meantone  temperam«&i. 
and  the  numbers  below  them  are  the  cents  in  the  intervals  from  c  to  these  notes. 

This  keyboard  is  applicable  to  any  intonation  in  which  all  notes,  by  the  neglect 
of  the  comma  or  skhisma,  are  reduced  to  one  set  of  Fifths,  no  matter  whether  pe*- 
fect  or  imperfect,  as  the  flat  Fifths  of  the  meantone  temperament.  On  referring 
to  the  plan,  we  see  how  the  53  division  is  placed  on  the  notes.    Mr.  Bosanqnct 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


SECT.  P. 


MR.  PAUL  WHITE'S  HARMON. 


481 


finds  it  convenient  to  use  7  x  12  =  84  digitals,  so  that  there  are  repetitions  as 
shewn  by  the  figures  at  the  bottom  of  the  plan,  each  of  the  12  columns  contain- 
ing 7  digitals.  The  position  of  the  meantone  notes  is  shewn  by  a  small  thick 
Roman  letter  at  the  top  of  the  digital  Having  36  digitals  at  his  disposal,  Mr. 
Bosanquet  has  used  36  notes  of  the  meantone  scale  in  place  of  only  27.  They 
are  disposed  in  12  rows  with  three  digitals  in  each  row.  In  the  plan,  thick  lines 
limit  tiie  three  digitals  thus  placed  at  the  disposal  of  the  meantone  notes,  and 
nnder  the  name  of  each  note  is  inserted  the  number  of  cents  in  the  interval  be- 
tween it  and  c.  It  will  thus  be  seen  that  each  note  differs  from  the  one  above  it 
in  the  plan  by  a  Didsis  of  41  cents.  Thus  the  first  row  has  bjf  1159,  c  o  =  1200, 
dt^  41.  Also  each  digital  lies  against  two  to  the  right  and  two  to  the  left.  The 
apper  one  to  the  right  is  76  cents  or  a  small  meantone  Semitone  higher,  the 
Lower  is  117  cents  or  a  great  meantone  Semitone  higher.  The  upper  one  to  the 
Left  is,  on  the  contrary,  117  cents  lower,  and  the  lower  one  to  the  left  is  76  cents 
Lower.  The  sum  of  the  two  Semitones  is  193  cents  =  ^(204  +  182),  a  Meantone,  f 
uid  their  difference  41  cents  a  Diesis. 

The  use  of  this  fingerboard  is  easily  acquired  by  any  pianist,  the  fingering  for 
idl  major  keys  resembling  that  for  A  major  on  ordinary  instruments. 

9.  Mb.  J.  Paul  White's  Habmon. 

Mr.  James  Paul  White,  of  Springfield,  Massachusetts,  U.S.  America,  a  tuner 
t)y  profession,  having  been  much  impressed  by  Mr.  Poole's  papers  in  Silliman*s 
Tou/mal,  cited  under  No.  7,  determined  to  realise  them  so  far  as  possible  by  means 
)f  the  53  division  of  the  Octave.  Now  on  examining  this  division  by  the  tables  in 
sect.  A.  art.  27,  and  sect.  D.  table  i,  we  find  that  the  number  of  degrees  by  which 
uiy  interval  is  represented  can  always  be  expressed  by  multiples,  or  the  sums  or 
lifferences  of  multiples,  of  2,  5,  7  (which  may  therefore  be  called  iridices),  as  in  the 
following  table : — 


Name  of  Interval 

ItsCenta 

Bepresented  In  the  53  dlylsion  by 

IT 

Cents 

Degrees 

Comma 

221 
24/ 

Pythagorean  Ck>mma 

23 

I 

=2x4-7 

Great  Diesis    .        .        .        . 

43 

n 

2 

=  2 

Small  Semitone 

70 

3 

=5x2-7 

Limma 

Greater  Limma 

92/ 

91 

4 

=  2x2 

Diatonic  Semitone  . 

112 

113 

5 

«5 

Minor  Tone     .... 

182 

181 

8 

=  2x4 

Major  Tone      .... 

204 

204 

9 

■5+2x2 

*  Snpermajor  Tone     . 

231 

226 

10 

=  5x2 

t  Snbminor  Third 

267 

272 

12 

«7  +  5 

Pythagorean  minor  Third 

294 

294 

13 

=7+2x3 

Jnst  minor  Third     . 

316 

317 

14 

«7X2 

Just  major  Third     . 

386 

s 

17 

-.7  +  5>^a 

Pythagorean  major  Third     '  . 

408 

18 

-7x2+2x2 

*  Supermajor  Third   . 

III 

430 

19 

-7x2+5 

f 

Foturth 

498 

22 

=7+5x3 

JastTritone    •        .        .        . 

590 

589 

26 

=7x3+5 

Pythagorean  Tritone 

612 

611 

27 

=7+5x4 

Grave  Fifth     .        .        .        . 

680 

679 

30 

-5x6=7x5-5 

Just  Fifth        .        .        .        . 

702 

702 

31 

=  7x3  +  5'<2 

Pythagorean  minor  Sixth 

792  \ 
794/ 

793 

Extreme  Sharp  Fifth 

35 

«7x5 

Jnst  minor  Sixth     . 

814 

815 

36 

=7x3+5x3=53-5x2-7 

Jast  major  Sixth     . 

884 

883 

39 

-7x5+2x2-53-7x2 

Jnst  diininished  Seventh 
•  Supermajor  Sixth    . 

926\ 
933/ 

928 

4X 

=  53-7-5 

Jnst  snperflnous  Sixth     . 

951 

42 

-7x6 

t  Snbminor  Seventh   . 
Extreme  sharp  Sixth 
Minor  Seventh 

976} 

974 

43 

'-53-5x2 

996 

996 

44 

=7x6+a 

Acate  minor  Seventh 

1018 

1019 

45 

=  5x9=53-2x4 

Jnst  major  Seventh . 

1088 

1087 

48 

=  53-5 

Pythagorean  major  Seventh    . 

mo 

1 109 

49 

=  7x7-53-2x2 

Octave     ...... 

1200 

1200 

53 

-7x4+5x5 

?Ie 



ninitboH '^>' ^    -»00 

482  ADDITIONS  BY  THE  TEANSLATOR.  app.xx. 

It  is  really  Biirprising  how  accurately  the  intervals  of  jnst  intonation  are  ihtis 
represented.  Only  those  marked  *  and  t  depending  on  the  7th  harmonic  are 
5  cents  too  flat,  or  sharp  respectively,  which  is  barely  perceptible. 

Influenced  no  doubt  by  such  a  calculation  as  the  above,  Mr.  Paul  White  con- 
ceived and  executed  a  fingerboard  of  which  the  typographical  plan  below  will 
give  some  conception.  And  this  conception  will  be  much  improved  by  drawing 
pencil  lines  on  the  diagram  parallel  to  the  rows  of  figures  sloping  up  (as  48  i  6 
II  16  21  26  31  36  41  46)  and  down  (as  41  48  2  9  16  23  30  37  44)  to  the  right. 
These  lines  will  divide  the  plan  into  a  number  of  parallelograms  or  irregdar 
lozenges,  each  of  which  represents  a  digital  of  nearly  the  same  shape,  but  fitting 
loosely  into  its  place.  These  are  pieces  of  wood  all  diamond-shaped  and  all  of 
the  same  height,  variously  marked  to  assist  the  player,  and  all  bearing  upon  them 
the  numbers  printed  in  the  plan.  The  typographical  plan  is,  of  course,  only 
approximatively  correct.  In  reality  the  vertical  lines  are  not  quite  vertical,  and  the 
lines  parallel  to  the  numbers  differing  by  12  as  39— 51  — 10— 22— 34— 46— 5  at 
U  the  top  (which  may  be  connected  by  pencil  lines  as  just  shewn)  are  more  nearly 
horizontal  or  rather  slope  slightly  downwards  instead  of  up.  But  as  the  design 
has  not  been  published,  it  was  desirable  to  give  only  a  conception  and  not  an 
accurate  plan  of  the  arrangement  with  the  curious  slopes  of  the  actual  lines. 

TYPOGRAPHICAL  PLAN  OP   MB.   J.   PAUL  WHITE'S  FINGEKBOABD. 

II39     .  5 

||27  46  12 

!|iS  34  83 

22  41  "^^        7    ■    , 

10  29  48  14 

51  fi7  36  2 

H     39  5  24  *t43  *t9 

46  12  t31  50  x6 

t53  19  38  4 

41  ~°        7  *26  45  " 

4^  14  33  *52  18 

2  ♦21  ♦«  6 

43  *8  28  47  *I3 

50  16  ♦35  1  20 

♦4  23  42  8 

45       .  n  *30  **9  15 

52  *18  37  3 

6  25  44 

f      47  *i3  32 

I  20  39 

8  27 

49  15 

3 

The  thick  figures  represent  white  digitals  and  serve  as  land-marks,  forming  the 
53  c,  gdy  18  e,  22/,  27/tt,  31  gr,  40  a,  496. 

The  numbers  marked  *  are  the  same  as  those  in  the  plan  of  Mr.  Bosanquet's, 
supr&,  p.  480,  and  represent  the  duodene  of  note  4.  The  numbers  marked  %  or 
53»  ^7»  3^>  43»9»  represent  the  chord  of  the  major  Ninth  c  «,  gr^i[>  d. 

In  the  columns  the  numbers  as  they  proceed  from  top  to  bottom  increase 
by  2,  of  course  taken  in  the  reverse  direction  they  decrease  by  2.  When  necefisary 
53  is  subtracted  or  added  here  and  elsewhere,  as  the  numbers  must  not  exceed  S3- 


Digitized  by  V^JiOOQlC  a 


SECT.  (I.  ON  TUNING  AND  INTONATION.  483 

In  the  lines  which  slope  wp  to  the  right  the  numhers  increase  upwards  hy  5,  and 
of  course  decrease  downwards  by  5. 

In  the  lines  which  slope  down  to  the  right  the  numbers  increase  downwards 
by  7,  and  of  course  decrease  upwards  by  7. 

It  is  thus  seen  that  the  three  indices  2,  5,  7  are  represented  by  nearly  vertical 
and  sloping  lines,  and  it  becomes  easy  by  the  preceding  table  to  pick  out  any  intervaL 
Thus  to  take  the  just  minor  Sixth  from  18  (one  of  the  thick  figures) ;  we  have  by 
the  table,  36 =7x3  +  5x3,  so  that  we  go  down  3  steps  on  the  line  of  7's,  and  then  up 
3  steps  on  the  line  of  5's,  and  thus  reach  i,  the  right  degree  for  18  +  36=54=53  +  1. 
!Biit  in  the  table  we  also  find  36=53  —  5x2  —  7,  hence  we  may  also  go  down 
2  steps  on  the  line  of  5's  and  then  up  1  step  on  the  line  of  7's,  reaching  i  as  before, 
but  not  the  same  i.  It  is  now  the  Octave  below,  and  if  from  this  new  i,  we  de- 
scend 4  steps  on  the  line  of  7's  and  then  ascend  5  steps  on  the  line  of  5's,  we  reach 
the  old  I,  for  7x4  +  5x5=28+25=53,  or  the  Octave. 

The  body  of  each  digital  is  a  block  of  wood  2^  inches  high  and  not  far  from  ^ 
I  inch  square  on  the  top.  The  grain  of  the  wood  is  vertical  so  as  to  feusilitate  the 
action  of  the  key  on  its  two  steel  guide  pins,  which  are  driven  firmly  into  a  board 
as  wide  as  the  manual.  The  valve  is  opened  by  a  pin  under  the  key  in  the  usual 
way.  Of  course  the  fingering  is  entirely  different  from  ordinary  fingering,  but  is 
the  same  in  all  possible  keys.  Contrasting  his  board  with  Mr.  Bosanquet's,  which 
he  admits  is  admirable  and  would  be  probably  regarded  with  more  favour  by 
musicians  than  his,  Mr.  Paul  White  (in  a  private  letter  to  me)  says  i)  that  his 
board  combines  the  advantages  of  Mr.  Poole's  with  Mr.  Bosanquet's,  and  has 
digitals  of  a  simpler  construction  than  either,  shewing  also  the  Pythagorean  tones 
conspicuously  for  every  note,  and  having  the  complete  cycle  of  tones.  2)  The 
chords  are  all  easy  for  the  fingers,  including  those  depending  on  the  7th  harmonic. 
3)  Digitals  differing  by  one  comma  are  far  apart,  so  that  there  is  no  danger  of 
playing  too  sharp  or  too  flat  by  a  comma.  The  fingers  can  easily  make  the  just 
chords,  but  to  make  them  false  by  a  comma  is  difficult.  4)  This  fingerboard  can 
be  made  more  compact  than  any  other.  The  extreme  width  of  the  present  in- 
strument (the  third  made)  is  only  1 1  inches,  or  twice  the  ordinary  width.  'If 

Mr.  Paul  White  uses  only  56  digitals  to  the  Octave,  Nos.  15,  27,  39,  marked  [|, 
being  the  only  repeats.  He  was  kind  enough  to  send  me  two  photographs  of  his 
instrimient,  (which  he  calls  the  Harmon,  and  which  he  constructed  almost  entirely 
with  his  own  hands,)  one  giving  a  bird's-eye  view  of  the  digitals,  and  the  other  their 
connection  with  the  rods  that  open  the  valves.  He  has  as  yet  not  arranged  any 
system  of  notation  and  does  not  himself  play  on  his  instrument  from  notes. 

SECTION  G. 

ON  TUNING  AND   INTONATION. 

(See  notes  pp.  256,  287,  31 1,  325.) 
Art.  Art. 

1.  Difficulties  of  tuning,  p.  483.  10.  The  '  Tuning   Octave  *    for   harmonium, 

2.  Specimens  of  tuning  in  meantone  tempera-  organ,  and  piano,  p.  488. 

ment,  p.  484.  11.  The  Translator's  Rule  for  tuning  in  equal  f 

3.  The  Fifths  and  major  Thirds  in  the  same,  temperament  beginning  with  c',  p.  489. 

p.  484.  12.  Modification  of  the  same  for  beginning 

4.  Specimens  of  tuning  in  equal  tempera-  with  a',  p.  489. 

ment,  p.  485.  13.  Proof  of  the  Bule,  p.  490. 

5.  Examination  of  the  Fifths  and  Fourths  in       14.  Bule  for  checking  the  tuning  of  Octaves, 

four  of  the  same,  p.  485.  p.  491. 

6.  Violin  Intonation  according  to  the  observa-  15*  The  Translator's  Bule  for  tuning  in  mean- 
tions  of  Comu  and  Mercadier,  p.  486.  tone  temperament,  p.  491. 

7.  Observations  on  the  same,  p.  487.  16.  Mr.  Bosanquet's  method  of  tuning  of  the 

8.  Scheibler's  method  of  tuning,  p.  488.  53  division,  p.  492. 

9.  The  Translator's   approximative   method  17.  Mr.  J.  Paul  White's  two  metliods  of  tuning 
and  counting  beats,  p.  488.  the  same,  p.  492. 

Art.  I. — We  have  seen  in  sect.  E.  that  just  tertian  harmony  requires  the  dis- 
crimination of  1 17  different  tones  within  the  Octave.  They  all  indeed  depend  upon 
just  Fifths,  Fourths,  Thirds,  and  Sixths.  But  very  few  ears  could  be  trusted  to 
tune  a  succession  of  perfect  Fifths  and  Fourths.  Herr  G.  Appunn  told  me  that  it 
cost  him  an  immense  labour  to  tune  36  notes  forming  perfect  Fifths  and  Fourthsj 

Digitized  by  WQOQ  IC 


484 


ADDITIONS  BY  THE  TRANSLATOR. 


APP.  XI. 


upon  an  experimental  harmonium,  and  he  had  the  finest  ear  for  appreciating 
intervals  that  I  have  ever  heard  of.  The  accumulation  of  ahnost  insensible  into 
intolerable  errors  besets  all  attempts  to  tune  by  a  long  series  of  similar  intervals. 
Even  Octaves  are  rarely  tuned  accurately  through  the  compass  of  a  grand  pianoforte. 
But  for  major  Thirds  and  minor  Sixths  there  is  no  chance  at  all  (except  by  a  real 
piece  of  haphazard  luck)  to  get  even  one  interval  tuned  with  absolute  correctness 
by  mere  appreciation  of  ear.  Hence  to  attempt  to  tune  the  Duodenarium  of 
sect.  E.  art.  18,  p.  463,  merely  by  Fifths  and  major  Thirds  is  quite  hopeless. 
But  if  we  cannot  tune  just  intervals  with  sufficient  correctness,  how  can  we  expect 
to  tune  all  the  variously  tempered  intervals  mentioned  in  sect.  A.. (and  these  are 
only  a  few  of  the  most  important)  sufficiently  well  to  discriminate  their  qualities 
and  appreciate  their  merits  ?  No  ear  knows  d  priori  what  result  it  has  to  expect, 
or  has  any  means  of  judging  whether  the  result  obtained  is  correct.  It  follows 
that  all  attempts  to  tune  by  ear  must  have  grievously  failed,  wherever  they  de- 
pended upon  considerable  alterations  of  just  intervals,  and  that  even  the  laborious 
IF  and  careM  training  of  modem  tuners  for  obtaining  the  very  slightly  altered  Fifths 
and  Fourths  of  equal  temperament  can  only  lead  tibem  to  absolute  correctness  '  by 
accident.' 

Art.  2. — To  ascertain  whether  these  theoretical  views  were  confirmed  in  prac- 
tice, I  have  made  some  observations  on  the  tuning  of  the  old  meantone  and  the 
recent  equal  temperaments.  It  is  easy  (from  the  data  in  sect.  A.)  to  determine 
the  cents  which  should  be  contained  in  each  interval,  and  (by  measuring  the  actual 
pitch  of  each  note  with  the  forks  described  on  p.  4466')*  ^  ^^^  what  the  interval 
obtained  in  any  particular  case  really  is.  For  brevity  I  give  only  the  names  of  the 
notes  in  the  octave,  and  the  interval  in  cents  from  the  lowest  note.  But  every 
such  figure  is  the  result  of  a  careful  observation. 


Line  i  in  the  following  Table  gives  the 
theoretical  number  of  cents. 

Line  2  gives  the  cents  observed  on  a  pitch- 
pipe  of  1730  belonging  to  the  bellfonndry 
Golbaochini  at  Padua,  blown  with  the  least 
^  force  of  wind  possible  to  bring  out  the  tone, 
on  an  organ  bellows  at  Mr.  T.  Hill's,  the  organ- 
builder's. 

Line  3  gives  those  observed  on  another 
pitch-pipe  of  1780,  belonging  to  the  same  and 
similarly  blown.  The  mean  value  of  a'  on 
both  was  425*2  vib. 


Line  4  gives  those  observed  on  accurate 
copies  of  a  set  of  timing-forks  (V  and  &'  miss- 
ing) belonging  to  the  beUfoundry  Gavedini  at 
Verona,  supposed  to  be  a  century  old,  and  pre- 
served with  great  care,  having  a'  423-2. 

Line  5  gives  the  cents  from  an  octave  oi 
pipes  on  Green's  organ  at  St.  Katharine's. 
Begent's  Park,  from  the  pitches  determined  b; 
me  in  1878,  up  to  which  time  it  was  one  oi 
the  few  organs  tuned  in  meantone  tempera- 
ment. Of  course  in  this  case  the  tuning  was 
modem. 


Specimens  of  Tuning 

in  Meantone  Temperament. 

Notes 

I 

c 
0 

CZ 

D 

E\> 

E 

F 

n 

G 

n 

A 

B\> 

B 

c 

76 

193 

310 

386 

503 

579 

697 

in 

890 

1007 

10^3 

1200 

2 

0 

102 

216 

308 

414 

516 

619 

723 

827 

920 

103 1 

1 126 

1232 

3 

0 

134 

236 

329 

443 

561 

626 

701 

814 

900 

982 

I07S 

"75 

4 

0 

"7 

229 

301 

439 

507 

633 

733 

864 

934 

1042 

— 

5 

0 

72 

198 

300 

3«2 

509 

586 

699 

78s 

898 

IQ20 

1096 

1209 

Art.  3.  Meantone  Fifths,  if  properly  tuned, 
should  have  696*6  cents,  and  the  major  Thirds 
386*3  cents.  The  old  tuners  did  not  use  the 
Fourth  in  tuning,  but  took,  for  example,  d  to 
(f^  then  ^  to  d'\  and  then  the  Octave  down  to 
d\  thence  the  Fifths  d!  to  a\  a!  to  e",  and  the 
Octave  down  to  e',  so  that  c'  to  e'  ought  to  be 
a  just  major  Third.  The  Fifths  and  major 
Thirds  actually  obtained  can  be  calculated 
from  the  Table  in  Art.  2  by  subtraction,  taking 
care  to  increase  the  minuend  by  1200  when  it 
is  less  tiian  the  subtrahend.  We  thus  find  the 
following  values.  The  figures  placed  between 
the  names  of  any  two  notes  give  the  cents  in 
the  interval  between  them,  which,  neglecting 
decimals,  should  be  697  for  Fifths,  and  386  for 
major  Thirds. 

Line  2.  Old  Pitch-pipe.  Fifths:  J?b723Bb 


685  F  684  C  723  G^  693  D  704  4  694  JB  712  B 

693  -^  683  c«  725  <n  • 

Major  Thirds :  E\>  415  (?  403  B ;  Bb  385  D 
403  1?^ ;  F404  ^  382  CI ;  C  414  -B  413  Gl  - 

Line  3.  Another  old  Pit<^-pipe.  Fifths: 
E\>  653  Bb  779  ^  614  C  701  G  735  D  664  J 
743  E  632  B  751  Ft  708  Cf  780  Ql . 

Major  Thirds:  JSb  372  G  374  B;  Bb  454  1> 
390  Fl ;  ^  339  ii  434  C«  ;  C  443  ^  471  G5  . 

Line  4.  Old  Forks.  Fifths :  £7b  741  fib 
665  JP  693  C  733  Gf  696  D  705  A  705  K\ 
(B  missing) ;  ^  684  C](  747  <it  • 

Major  Thirds :  J?b  432  O ;  (B  missing) ;  fib 
387 B4042?'«;lr'427 .438305;  CaV^E^^(^. 

These  old  tunings  are  very  imperfect.  BoUi 
Fifths  and  major  Thirds  would  make  dreadful 
harmony.  The  forks  are  if  anything 
than  the  pitch-pipes. 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


^TECT.  O. 


ON  TUNING  AND  INTONATION. 


435 


Line  5.  St  Katharine's  Organ.  Fifths:  Eb 
^20  JBb  6891^7000  699  G  699  D  700.1  684 -fi? 
^  14  B  690  i?Tr  686  Cff  713  On . 

Major  Thirds :  ^b  399  G  397  B ;  Bb  378  D 
3S8  Ft;  Fz^A  374  C« ;  C  382  ^  403  Gfff . 

Thia  modem  tuning  is  better,  but  not  good. 

Art.  4. — It  takes  a  quick  man  three  years  to  learn  bow  to  tune  a  piano  well 
ixL  equal  temperament  by  estimation  of  ear,  as  I  learn  from  Mr.  A.  J.  Hipkins. 
Timers  bave  not  time  for  any  otber  metbod.    Tbe  following  are  good  examples : — 

liine  I.  The  theoretical  intervals,  all  ezaot 
bundreds  of  cents. 

liine  2.  My  own  piano,  tuned  by  one  of 
JBroadwoods*  usual  tuners,  and  let  stand  un- 
used for  a  fortnight. 

liines  3,  4,  5.  Three  grand  pianos  by 
Sroadwoods'  best  tuners,  prepared  for  examin- 
flktion  through  the  kindness  of  Mr.  A.  J.  Hip- 
luns,  of  that  house. 

Line  6.  An  organ  tuned  a  week  previously 
t>y  one  of  Mr.  T.  Bill's  tuners,  and  used  only 


The  Fifths,  which  should  be  a  quarter  of  a 
comma  5*4  cents  flat,  are  often  sharp;  the 
major  Thirds  are  unequal.  But  the  errors 
here  are  not  more  than  might  be  reasonably 
expected  from  tuning  by  ear. 


once,  examined  by  the  kind  permission  of  Mr. 
G.  Hickson,  treasurer  of  South  Place  Chapel, 
Finsbury,  where  the  organ  stood. 

Line  7.  An  harmonium  tuned  by  one  of 
Messrs.  Moore  A  Moore's  tuners,  kindly  pre- 
pared for  mj  examination. 

Line  8.  An  harmoniimi,  used  as  a  standard  ^ 
of  "pitch,  tuned  a  year  previously  by  Mr.  D.  J. 
Blaikley    (p.   97^),  by  means  of  accurately 
counted  beats,  <&c.,  with  a  constant  blast,  put 
at  my  disposal  for  examination  by  Mr.  Blaikley. 


Specimens  of  Tuning  in  Equal  Temperament. 

Notes 

<^ 

cz 

D 

200 

300 

E 
400 

F 

Ft 

G 

('^ 

A 

At 

B 

c 

I 

0 

100 

500 

600 

700 

800 

900 

1000 

1 100 

1200 

2 

0 

96 

197 

297 

392 

49« 

S90 

700 

797 

894 

990 

1089 

1 201 

3 

0 

99 

200 

305 

4x1 

497 

602 

707 

805 

902 

XOO3 

1 102 

1206 

4 

0 

100 

200 

300 

395 

502 

599 

702 

800 

«97 

999 

IIOO 

1200 

5 

0 

lOI 

199 

299 

399 

500 

598 

696 

800 

899 

999 

1 100 

1200 

6 

0 

lOI 

192 

297 

399 

502 

601 

702 

806 

898 

1005 

1099 

I20I 

7 

0 

98 

200 

298 

396 

49« 

599 

702 

800 

898 

999 

1099 

1199 

8 

0 

100 

200 

300 

399 

499 

600 

700 

800 

900 

1001 

1099 

X200 

These  were  all  tuned  by  the  modem  way  of 
Fifths  up  and  Fourths  down,  and  the  object  is 
to  make  the  Fifth  up  2  cents  too  close,  and 
the  Fourth  down  2  cents  too  open.  As  this 
interval  of  2  cents  lies  on  the  very  boundary  of 
perception  by  ear,  the  difficulty  of  tuning  thus 
without  attending  to  the  beats  is  enormous. 
The  above  figures  in  lines  2,  3,  4,  5  shew  how 
very  close  an  approximation  is  now  possible  in 
pianofortes. 

Art.  5. — The  order  of  tuning  differs  in  dif- 
ferent houses.  Messrs.  Moore  &  Moore's  tuners 
set  </  by  a  c'*  fork,  and  then  tune  in  order : 
tf  g  d'  aef  bft  dt  gt  d't  •  Then  begin  again 
and  go  on  as  c'  /  6  b  e'b.  The  proof  of  the  work 
is  that  e'b  and  d%  are  identical.  Messrs. 
Broadwoods'  tuners  also  set  &  from  c,  but  then 
proceed  thus :  cfgd'ae'bfZ  c'%  gt  d*Z  atfc^, 
the  proof  being  that  the  final  agrees  with  the 
initial  cf.    In  this  case  a%f  is  taken  m  bbf 


f 

or  at  et  t  that  is  a  Fourth  down.  Observe  that 
the  tuning  in  both  cases  takes  place  in  the 
Octave /to/,  for  which  the  beats  of  disturbed 
Fifths  and  even  of  disturbed  Fourths  are  very 
slow.  This  arises  from  the  great  prominence 
of  the  second  partial  tone  in  this  region  on 
pianoforte  notes.  In  taking  the  pitch  of  each 
note,  I  found  that  d',  d't ,  e'  taken  as  disturbed 
unisons,  beat  with  my  forks  much  less  dis- 
tinctly than  /,  ft ,  &c.,  to  eft  fts  disturbed 
Octaves.  Now  the  above  table  enables  us  to 
calculate  the  cents  in  the  Fifths  and  Fourths 
actually  tuned,  which  were  the  intervals  esti- 
mated by  ear.  I  take  only  line  i  as  containing 
the  theoretical  intervals,  and  lines  2  to  5  as 
being  by  Broadwoods'  tuners,  so  that  the  order 
is  certain.  The  numbers  of  cents  placed  be- 
tween two  notes  shews  the  interval,  all  the  ^ 
Fourths  being  ti^en  down  and  the  Fifths  up. 


Pianoforte  Tuning — Fourths  and  Fifths. 


I    c'500    0700    d'soo    ayoo    e'500    6500   /« 700    c'ffsoo    (75700    d'JJ  500    ©5500   /700    c' 


2 

500 

697 

503 

1698 

3 

493 

693 

498 

1709 

4 

498 

698 

503 

;698 

5 

504 

703 

500 

I700 

503 

499 

706 

499 

700 

507 

497 

703 

509 

506 

697 

594 

708 

502 

506 

709 

495 

501 

701 

500 

700 

50X 

*97 

698 

500 

502 

703 

501 

700 

SOI 

49« 

700 

These  examples  must  probably  be  con- 
sidered the  best  that  pianofoi-te  tuning  by  ear 
can  accomplish.  But  even  in  line  5,  which  is 
the  best,  there  are  only  five  intervals  abso- 
lutely correct,  two  others  are  only  an  inappre- 
ciable I  cent  in  error,  two  are  a  just  appreciable 
2  cents  wrong,  two  are  3  cents  out,  and  one 


wrong  by  the  very  pjerceptible  interval  of  4 
cents.  Now  if  this  is  the  work  of  a  clever 
tuner  in  constant  practice  for  many  hours  daily 
for  many  years,  in  tuning  one  kind  of  tem- 
perament only,  what  are  we  to  expect  from 
those  who  attempt  to  realise  new  intervals  ? 


Digitized  by 


Google 


486 


ADDITIONS  BY  THE  TRANSLATOR. 


APP.  XX, 


Art.  6. — ^Tlie  vocalist  does  not,  properly  spealdng,  tnne  at  all.  It  is  with  him 
a  matter  of  ear,  that  is,  sense  of  pitch,  which  guides  the  muscles  to  alter  the  tension 
of  his  vocal  chords,  and  make  them  produce  tones  of  various  pitch.  The  ease  and 
rapidity  with  which  this  can  b^  done  are  matters  of  careful  training,  followed  by 
long  practice.  They  can  never  be  acquired  by  those  who  have  not  the  proper 
cerebral  organisation.  The  extreme  mobility  of  the  voice  and  the  difficulty  of  sus- 
taining a  pitch,  or  of  exactly  reacliing  it  again  after  a  pause,  throw  great  impedi- 
ments in  the  way  of  testing  unaccompanied  singers.  The  habit  of  choral  singing 
leads  to  just  intonation  (App.  XVIII.),  but  an  accompanying  instrument  is  quite 
sufficient  to  lead  the  voice  astray  (p.  207,  note  f).  Hence  I  pass  by  voices  alto- 
gether. The  violinist  apparently  tunes  only  four  strings  to  make  three  perfect 
Fifths,  and  in  doing  so  he  is  assisted  by  an  audible  combinational  tone,  which  should 
be  just  one  Octave  below  the  lower  note  of  the  pair  he  is  tuning,  and  hence  a  Twelfth 
below  the  upper  note.  But  he  really  tunes  every  note  in  the  compass  of  his  instro- 
ment  g  to  e"",  by  his  method  of  stopping,  as  much  as  the  pianoforte  tuner  by 
H  increasing  or  diminishing  the  tension  of  his  strings.  And  according  to  the 
'  school,'  he  should  tune  the  notes  in  equal  temperament.  How  then  does  be 
tune  ?  Or,  to  put  the  question  in  more  usual  language,  what  intonation  does  he 
use  ?  Messrs.  Comu  and  Mercadier  (as  mentioned  on  p.  z^S^>  ^^^^  '^)  instituted 
a  series  of  experiments  on  voices  and  organs  by  the  phonautograph  {Comptei 
Rendus,  vol.  Ixviii.  pp.  301  and  427),  and  on  violins  and  violoncellos  by  means  of  a 
tin  plate  placed  under  the  bridge,  which  was  connected  with  a  wire  that  conducted 
the  vibrations  to  the  inscribing  style  (see  Comptes  Benchis,  17  July  1871,  vol. 
Ixxiii.  p.  178,  from  which,  and  vol.  Ixxiv.  p.  321,  vol.  Ixxvi.  p.  432, 1  obtained  the 
data  which  I  have  here  reduced  to  cents).  I  give  the  results  only  for  different  indi- 
vidual pljiyers  on  the  violin  and  violoncello.  Some  of  the  scales  are  fragmentary. 
I  prefix  the  just,  Pythagorean,  and  equal  number  of  cents  for  comparison.  The 
scales  are  first  major  and  then  minor.  'The  root  is  omitted  as  unnecessary.  The 
numbers  in  one  line  refer  to  a  single  trial 


Scale  of  G  Major. 


Notes 

J> 

498 

0 

A  \    B         c     j 

Notes 

J> 

B 

F    a  \  A 

i^  1  <  1 

Jnst  oents 

204 

702 

884'io88,i20O 

Just  oents 

204 

386|498i702!884 

I088I200 

Pyth.  „ 

204 

408  498  702  906 1 1 10 1200; 

Pyth.  „ 

204408  498  702  906  1 1 10 1200 

Equal  ,♦ 

200 

400*500 

700900 
701 

1 100 

1200 

Equal  „ 

200 

400500 

700 

900!  1 100 

1200 

VioUn 

401 505 

Violin 

41149^ 

Amateurs 

212 

4121498,709 

Professional 

417495714] 

212 

399,499710' 

(M.  Leonard, 

417 

198 

396    1708 

Belgian) 

411 

2iO;4o6'5o5'7o2 

207 

910 

1 128 

188  401 1      I702 

216 

912 

1 122 

201 

4064901711 

II28 

1 

411 

490I       1 

207 

407 

II23 

201 

396 

495  70S 

411 

194 

411 

411 
404 

704 

207 

401 
401 
401 

899 

199 

407 

696 

209 

404 

1 
1 

Violoncello 

415 

710, 

Violoncello 

393 

487702 

Amatenrs 

39S 

1 

Professional 

407I      1 

208397 

507 

710 

(M.  S^ligmann) 

419,496,716 

408 

411 

1 

411 

399 

491 702 

.401 

712 

496, 

407 

705 

414 

695892 

404 

1 

407 

696896 

204 

415 

5" 

701 

1 109]  1 202 

403 

697907 

201 

5" 

702 

1 105' 

i98;4i5;5oo 

I 

j 

207'4i4J498j7oi|       '11201202 
202'403'499j7i2j       >iii7i20i 

1 

Digitized  by  VjOOQlC 


SECT.  O. 


ON  TUNING  AND  INTONATION. 
Scale  of  C  Minor. 


487 


Kotea 

1^ 

e\, 

/ 

9 

ab 

b 

1200 

{              Notes 

d 

eb 

1/ 

9 
702 

814 

b 
1088 

1200 

Just  cents 

204 

3161498702 

8141088 

Just  cents 

204[3i6;498 

Pyth.  „ 

204J294!498|702 

792  1 1 10 

1200 

Pyth.  .. 

204I294  498^702 

792 

mo 

1 200 

Equal  „ 

aoo 

300 

500 

700 
697 

800  I  IOC 

1 

1200 
1208 

Equal  „ 

200 

300  500  700 
295494700 

80c 

8x0 

IIOO 

1200 

Violin 

292 

Violin 

199 

Amateora 

281 

708 

Professional 

2071311499 

701 

793 

i"3 

295 

702 

I2IO 

(M.  Ferrand,  of 

2041298503 

698 

793 

1116 

1201 

295 

the        Op^ra 

206 

306492 

700 

794 

294 

1 196 

Gomique) 

204 

298; 

301 

70s 

1206 

298 

702 

1203 

202 

300 

701 

196 

300 

s 

195 

292 

707 

292 

303 

705782 

783 

709 

802 

III8 

II96 

696798 

nil 

1209 

712 
704 

It 

III8 

IIOI 

1 197 

712 

783 

1 102 

Art.  7. — Messrs.  Cornu  and  Mercadier  conclude  finaUy  (ibid.  vol.  IxxvL  p.  434) 
that  :— 

*  Musical  intervals  belong  to  at  least  two  different  systems  of  different  values : 

*  i)  The  intervals  employed  in  melodies  which  have  no  modulations  agree 
with  those  of  the  Pythagorean  scale.  ^ 

*  2)  The  intervals  between  two  notes  sounded  together  in  chords,  the  basis  of 
harmony,  have  for  their  ratios  the  following  numbers :  2  for  Octave,  ^  Fifth, 
J  Fourth,  f  major  Third,  4  minor  Third,  f  major  Sixth,  |  minor  Sixth,  and 
J  Seventh,  where  the  Fourtn  and  Sixths  were  deduced  firom  observation  of  the 
Fifth  and  Thirds,  and  the  Seventh  from  the  dominant  chord.' 


Thus  for  nnaocompanied  harmony  of  two 
tones  (chords  more  than  two  tones  were  not 
tried)  just  intonation  dUme  was  used.  For 
melody  the  major  Thirds,  major  Sixths,  and 
especially  the  major  Sevenths  (leading  notes) 
were  much  sharpened,  and  the  minor  Thirds 
and  minor  Sixths  generally  much  flattened. 
Bat  did  this  arise  from  the  custom  of  equal 
temperament?  (as  M.  GuSroult  thinks,  i6u2. 
9  May  1870,  vol.  Ixx.p.  1037,  to  which  Messrs. 
Coma  and  Mercadier  replied,  on  30  May  1870, 
vol.  Ixx.  p.  1 1 70)  or  really  from  the  feeling  of 
Fifths  ?  The  latter  was  impossible  for  the  lead- 
ing note,  which  is  sometimes  much  sharper 
than  in  Pythagorean  intonation,  and  the  Fifths 
played  were  by  ho  means  always  true.  Messrs. 
Coma  and  Mercadier  say  that  the  divergence 
from  the  mean  only  reaches  ^  of  a  comma, 
that  is,  about  7  cents;  but  as  the  Pytha- 
gorean major  Sixth,  major  Third,  and  major 
Seventh  differ  from  the  corresponding  equal 
tempered  intervals  by  only  6,  8,  and  10  cents 
respectively,  this  uncertainty  renders  it  im- 
possible to  decide  whether  the  scale  played 
was  intentionally  equal  or  Pythagorean,  or 
whether  even  it  did  not  vary  with  the  feeling 
of  the  moment.  Taking  into  consideration 
that  the  pitches  actually  shewn  in  the  tables 
vary  considerably,  that  they  very  rarely  repeat 
themselves,  that  the  notes  are  sometimes 
flatter  and  sometimes  sharjier  than  either  just 


or  Pythagorean  intonation,  and  that  this  un- 
certainty pervades  even  such  intervals  as  the 
Fourth,  Fifth,  and  Octave,  I  am  inclined  to 
adopt  the  hypothesis  of  an  intentionally  vari- 
able intonation.  Whether  founded  on  the 
feeling  of  Pythagorean  or  equal  temperament, 
it  is  cUfficnlt  to  decide.  But  it  is  certainly  not 
founded  on  any  feeling  of  just  intonation  for 
harmony.  If  then  these  players,  as  Messrs. 
Cornu  and  Mercadier  assert  from  first  to  last 
in  the  unmistakable  terms  already  cited,  adopt 
just  intonation  of  intervals  for  harmony,  a  5[ 
serious  question  arises  as  to  how  they  treat 
the  relations  of  tonality.  The  first  part  may 
lead  and  the  others  may  be  adapted  to  it,  or 
the  bass  may  determine  the  intonation  of  the 
other  parts.  In  either  case  there  would  be  ^ 
great  variability,  through  which  modulation, 
and  even  the  adjustment  of  parts  without 
much  previous  combined  practice,  would  be- 
come extremely  difficult;  see  pp.  208c  and 
note  *,  324^),  c,  d.  But  how  about  the  return 
to  the  same  key  after  modulations  (p.  3286)  ? 
Huyghens  (Cosmothedros,  lib.  i.  p.  77,  as 
cited  by  Dr.  Smith,  Harmonics^  2nd  ed. 
p.  228)  suggests  that  as  *  erring  from  the  pitch 
first  assumed  .  .  .  would  greatly  offend  the 
ear  of  the  musician,  he  naturally  avoids  it  by 
his  memory  of  pitch,  and  by  tempering  the 
intervals  of  the  intermediate  sounds,  so  as  to 
return  to  it  again.'    But  ho^  accurately  does 


Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


488  ADDITIONS  BY  THE  TBANSLATOB.  app.  xx. 

he  remember  the  original  pitch  ?     In  some  a  series  of  modulations  be  noticed  ?    We  knofw 

oases  in  the  above  observations  of  Coma  and  indeed  that  unaccompanied  singers  constantly 

Mercadier  the  Octave  (and  hence  the  original  *  flatten  *  by  much  more  than  a  comma.     The 

pitch)  will  be  seen  to  be  sharpened  or  flattened  Duodenarium  simply  shews  what  sounds  ought 

by  more  than  2  cents.    In  the  course  of  a  few  to  be  played  in  modulations,  and  would  be 

modulations  this  2  cents  might  easily  become  played  on  instruments  with  fixed  tones  pio- 

22  or  a  comma.    Where  is  the  guarantee  for  perly  arranged ;  not  what  intervals  are  nom 

remembering  the  original  pitch?    Would  an  played  and  sung,  and  mere  memoj^  of  ear 

alteration  of  a  whole  comma  in  passing  through  does  not  suffice. 

Art.  8. — Sclieibler*s  method  of  timing  instruments  was  theoretically  perfect. 
It  consisted  in  tuning  by  his  fork  tonometer  a  series  of  forks  4  vib.  flatter  than  the 
pitches  required.  The  string,  pipe,  or  reed  was  then  tuned  sharper  than  the  fork  by 
4  beats  in  a  second.  (See  p.  4466  for  Eoenig's  forks  for  this  purpose.)  This  only 
applied  to  one  octave,  and  perhaps  the  octave  below ;  the  others  were  tuned  from 
them  by  estimation  of  Octave.    The  errors  thus  made  were  a  minimum,   but 

^  there  was  the  obvious  disadvantage  of  having  to  tune  a  new  set  of  forks  for  each 
pitch  desired.  This  Scheibler  overcame  by  tuning  auxiliary  octaves  on  organs,  ajid 
counting  beats  by  rather  a  troublesome  rule,  entailing  the  need  of  an  accurate 
metronome.  Even  then  he  only  taught  how  to  tune  in  equal  temperament.  For 
practical  purposes  we  require  not  only  the  equal  but  the  meantone  temperament, 
and  also  the  53  division  of  the  octave.  The  only  sure  way  is  by  calculating  the 
pitch  number  of  each  note,  and  thus  determining  the  beats  between  the  note  and 
the  forks  of  a  tuning-fork  tonometer.  This  method  may  be  dismissed  as  generally 
impracticable.  80  may  any  method  which  depends  upon  accurately  knowing  the 
pitch  number  of  the  tuning-note.  What  is  required  is  a  method  of  tuning  at  any 
unknown  pitch  which  an  organ  or  piano  may  happen  to  possess  at  the  moment 
within  the  limits  of,  say,  0^256  and  c'270  without  determining  exactly  what  that 
pitch  really  is.  This  would  save  the  great  trouble  of  entirely  altering  the  pitch 
of  the  piano  (never  very  certain  in  its  results),  and  the  still  greater  trouble  and 
expense  of  enturely  altering  the  pitch  of  an  organ  or  harmonium. 

Art.  9. — For  this  purpose  I  invented  an  approximative  method  (given  on  p.  785 

IT  of  the  ist  edition  of  this  translation  and  subsequently  communicated  to  the 
Musical  Times),  which  I  here  subjoin  in  an  improved  form.  It  is  based  on  the 
result  of  Prof.  Preyer's  investigations  (supn^  p.  147^^')  that  errors  of  ^  or  '2  of  a 
vibration  cannot  be  heard  in  any  part  of  the  scale,  so  that  any  attempt  to  tone 
more  accurately  is  labour  thrown  away.  Moreover,  even  at  high  pitches  '3,  *4,  and 
•5  vib.  are  scarcely  perceptible  in  melody  and  quite  inoffensive  in  harmony.  It 
will  be  found  very  difficult  when  the  beats  are  less  than  4  in  10  seconds,  that  is, 
when  the  error  is  less  than  '4  vib.,  to  count  them  with  any  approach  to  accuracy. 
But  it  is  only  by  beats  that  we  can  work  effectively. 

Any  one  who  undertakes  tuning  should  only,  there  are  10  doable  swings  in  10  seeonds. 

learn  to  estimate  the  meaning  of  6,  10, 15,  20,  By  watching  and  counting  this,  say  for  half  &n 

30  beats  in  10  seconds.    This  is  best  done  by  hour,  the  tuner  will  learn  to  feel  the  rate  of 

short  pendulums  eonstructed  of  a  piece  of  these  two  sets  of  swings.    Then  make  another 

thread  with  one  end  tied  to  a  curtain  ring,  and  pendulum  with  a  length  of  string  of  4}  inches 

the  oth^  passed  through  a  sUt  in  a  piece  of  measured  as  before.  This  vibrates  much  more 

firewood,  round  which  it  is  ultimately  tied,  quickly,  making  180   single   and   hence   90 

%  The  stick  is  put  under  a  book  by  the  side  of  a  double  swings  in  60  seconds,  and  consequently 

table,  so  that  the  pendulum  swings  freely.  30  single  and  15  double  swings  in  10  seconda. 

Measure  the  length  of  the  string  from  the  Finally  make  the  length  27^  inches,  always 

centre  of  the  ring  to  the  beginning  of  the  from  tiie  centre  of  the  ring  to  the  stick,  and 

wood,  which  aUows  of  an  easy  alteration  of  the  pendulum  will  make  6  'double  and   12 

the  length  by  drawing  the  string  through  the  single  vibrations  in  10  seconds.     Bemember 

slit.   Make  this  length  9|  inches.  The  pendu-  that  if  you  begin  counting  with  one,  you  will 

lum  swings  backwards  and  forwards  120  times  end  with  seven  for  6,  eleven  for  10,  uxteen 

in    60  seconds,  and  hence  20  times  in  10  for  15,  and  so  on,  so  that  you  will  always  have 

seconds.     Adjust    it   more  accurately  by  a  to  throw  oft  one  from  your  count, 
seconds  w«tchl    Counting  the  swings  one  way 

Art.  10. — The  rule  has  to  be  arranged  in  several  £orms  according  to  the  custom 
of  tuning  instruments.  Harmoniums  are  best  tuned  from  c*  to  c",  that  is,  in  the 
two-foot  octave.  Organs  are-  generally  tuned  in  the  principal  stop,  so  that  on 
touching  the  keys  from  &  to  c",  the  sounds  are  from  c"  to  &'\  in  the  one-foot 
octave,  and  hence  the  beats  are  twice  as  fast.  But  for  pianos  it  is  the  custom  to 
tune  from/  to/  (see  art.  5).    Most  tuners  in  England  begin  with  &\  from  which 


Digitized  by  VjOOQlC 


BBCT.  O. 


ON  TUNING  AND  INTONATION. 


489 


a'  is  '  set/  and  then  the  tuning  commences.  Some  tmiers  in  England  and  all 
al)road  begin  with  a!.  This  makes  no  difference  in  the  rule,  provided  the  tuning 
octave  remains  the  same. 


Absolately  the  beats  arising  from  imper- 
fect Fifths  and  Sixths  vary  for  every  difference 
of  pitch  of  the  lower  note.  As  the  Fifth  is 
always  too  dose  and  the  Fourth  too  open,  the 
reader  can  find  the  beats  from  the  numbers  in 
tlie  table,  p.  437c,  <2,  by  subtracting  twice  the 
larger  from  three  times  the  smaller  pitch 
nximber  for  Fifths  (thus  d  \  ^  in  col.  ii.  of  the 
table  is  258*6  :  387*6,  whence  3x258*6— 2  x 
387*6  =  775*8 -775*2 -'6,  giving  6  beats  in  10 


seconds),  and  four  times  the  smaller  pitch 
from  three  times  the  larger  for  Fourths  (thus 
^  '.d'"  387-6  :  290*3,  whence  3  x  387*6-4  x 
274B  1162*8  —  1161*2- 1*6  or  16  in  10  seconds). 
But  for  the  purposes  of  the  rule  all  the  beats 
of  Fifths  are  supposed  to  be  the  same  through- 
out the  tuning  octave,  and  similarly  all  the 
beats  of  Fourths  are  assumed  to  be  tiie  same. 
The  errors  will  be  found  to  oorrect  each  other, 
and  in  no  case  to  exceed  the  permissible  limits. 


Art.  1 1 . — Bide  for  tuning  m  equal  temperament  at  any  pitch  between  c'256  and 
c' 2  70-4. 

Tune  in  the  following  order,  making  the  Fifths  closer  and  the  Fourths  wider  f 
than  perfect.    The  numbers  between  the  names  of  the  notes  indicate  the  beats 
in  10  seconds. 

For  harmoniums : 

&  10  g^  15  d'  10  a'  15  e'  10  b'  15 /#  15  c%  10  g%  15  d'#  10  a%  15/ 

For  organs,  using  the  metal  principal,  sounding  thus  an  Octave  higher  than 
the  digitals  shew : 

c"  20  ^"  30  (i"  20  a"  30  e''  20  b"  30  f%  30  &t  20  ^"Jf  30  d'f  20  a"#  30/' 

For  pianofortes : 

c'  10  g  6  d'  10  a  6  e*  10  b  10  fjj^  6  c%  10  ^  6  d%  10  ajj!  10/ 


On  the  piano  the  beats  can  often  be  heard 
for  only  5  seconds,  and  then  the  beats  will  be 
3  and  5  in  5  seconds,  in  place  of  6  and  10  in 
10  seconds. 

In  the  two  first  cases  the  intervals  beating 
10  in  10  seconds  are  all  Fifths  up,  those  beat- 
ing 15  in  10  seconds  are  Fourths  down;  in  the 
last  case  the  Fifths  up  beat  only  6  times  in  10 
seconds,  and  the  Fourths  down  beat  10  times 
in  10  seconds. 

Tune  each  Fifth  as  accurately  just^  or 
-without  beats,  as  possible,  and  then  make  the 
interval  closer  by  flattening  the  upper  note 
-very  slightly  indeed  till  10  beats  are  heard  in 
10  seconds.  Then  from  the  Fifth  thus  reached 
tune  a  Fourth  down  as  accurately  just^  or 
-without  beats,  as  possible,  and  then  make  the 
interval  opener  by  flattening  the  lower  note 


very  slightly  till  15  beats  are  heard  in  10 
seconds.  From  the  note  thus  gained  proceed 
to  the  next  until  /  is  reached.  The  Fourth  /  tr 
to  d  is  not  tuned,  as  both  notes  have  been 
determined.  It  never  beats  faster  than  15  in 
10  seconds. 

If  the  Fifths  and  Fourths  are  not  brought 
to  be  as  nearly  as  possible  just  in  the  first  in- 
stance  the  tuner  can  never  be  sure  whether 
the  second  note  of  the  interval  is  too  sharp  or 
too  flat,  because  the  error  itself  is  too  small 
to  be  judged  of  with  accuracy  on  merely 
sounding  the  notes  in  succession,  and  the 
same  number  of  beats  would  result  whether 
we  had  sharpened  or  flattened  the  note,  but 
the  whole  scheme  would  be  entirely  frustrated 
if  the  interval  were  rendered  opener  instead  of 
doser  or  conversely. 


Art.  12. — If  it  is  preferred  to  commence  on  a',  set  a'  to  fork  and  proceed  to  e\ 
b'  up  to/',  &c.,  as  in  the  regular  scheme.  Stop  at  /'  and  begin  again  at  a',  and 
time  the  Fifth  a'  to  d'  down,  first  making  the  interval  just,  and  then  making  it  % 
closer  by  sharpemng  the  lower  note  till  10  beats  in  10  seconds  result.  Next  take 
d^  to  g'  a  just  Fourth  down,  and  then  make  the  interval  opener  hy  flattening  the 
lower  note  till  15  beats  are  heard  in  a  second.  Lastly,  from  this  g^  take  &  a  Fifth 
lower,  and  after  making  it  just,  render  the  interval  closer  by  sharpening  the  lower 
note  till  10  beats  in  10  seconds  are  heard.  This  modification  is  merely  an  adapta- 
tion of  the  general  principle  that  Fifths  are  to  be  closer  than  just,  and  Fourths 
opener  than  just. 


The  tuner  should  carefully  familiarise  him- 
self with  tuning  just  Fifths  and  Fourths,  with 
recognising  them  as  just  by  their  total  absence 
of  beats  in  this  2-foot  Octave,  and  by  feeling 
how  beats  arise  by  altering  either  of  the  notes 
either  way.  On  the  harmonium  this  is  easy, 
when  the  just  interval  has  been  tuned.  It  is 
only  necessary  to  press  down  the  digital  of  the 
upper  note  slightly,  so  as  just  to  hear  the 


note ;  this  process  flattens  it  and  renders  the 
interval  closer;  or  to  do  the  same  with  the 
lower  note,  which  renders  the  interval  opener. 
In  either  case  beats  ensue.  As  many  just 
Fourths  and  Fifths  exist  on  the  Harmonical, 
this  experiment  is  ready  to  hand. 

Art.  13. — The  proof  of  my  rule  consists  in 
shewing  that  for  c'256,  a'435,  a'440,  c'27o*4,  it 
leads  to  results  which  no  ear  could  distinguish 


Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


490 


ADDITIONS  BY  THE  TRANSLATOR. 


APP.  XX. 


ir 


from  perfectly  correct  tuning,  or  which  are 
equal  at  least  to  the  very  best  in  art.  4.  For 
it  is  clear  that  if  it  holds  for  all  these  pitches, 
of  which  one  is  practically  the  lowest  and  one 
practically  the  highest  in  use,  and  the  other 
two  are  as  nearly  as  may  be  halfway  between 
them,  it  must  hold  for  all  intermediate  pitches. 
Now  the  results  obtained  by  the  rule  are  easily 
calculated  for  a  given  c  or  a\  Using  c',  c'8  , 
d,\  ifec,  for  the  pitch  numbers  of  these  notes, 
and  remembering  that  the  rule  gives  i  beat  in 
a  second  for  flat  Fifths  and  1-5  beats  in  a 
second  for  sharp  Fourths,  we  have,  for  har- 
moniums or  organs,  supposing  d  known,  and 
the  above  rule  accurately  followed, 

2/    «3c'    -I 
4^'    =3/    -1*5 
2a!    =3d'    -I 


4c'    =3a'   -1-5 
26'    =  3e'  - 1 
4/ff  =3^>'   -1*5 
4c'ff  =3/«-i-5 
2(7'tt  =3c'5  -I 
A^'l  =  3^'tt  - 1'5 
2a'«=3^'«-i 
4/    =3flt'tt-r5. 


If  we  begin  with  a\  then  the  three  first  equa- 
tions will  give 

3d'  =  2a'+i,  3^'  =  4d'+i-5,  3c'  =  2/+i. 


In  the  case  of  the  pianoforte,  using  the  Octave 
/  to  /,  Uie  equations  for  calculating  the  pitches 
of  the  notes  from  beats  will  be,  if  we  begin 
with  c', 

4g     -3c'     -I 

2d    =3gf     -  -6 

4a    =  3d     —  I 


zd  ^Tfl,  —  -6 

46  ^yl  - 1 

4/B  «32»  -I 

2c'«  =3/tt  -  -6 

4{75  ^V^l  -* 

2d%  «3^  -  -6 

4flff  =3i'ff  -I 

4/  =3««  -I- 


But  if  we  begin  with  a,  the  first  three  equations 
will  give 

3(i  =  4a+i,  3^«2d  +  '6,  3c'«4<?+i. 

In  the  following  calculations  the  rule  is 
necessarily  supposed  to  be  carried  out  with 
perfect  accuracy.  Of  course  in  practice,  espe- 
cially when  the  beats  are  estimated  instead  of 
being  accurately  counted,  this  is  impossible. 
But  the  results  will  be  found  much  more  accu- 
rate than  in  the  ordinary  way  of  tuning  en- 
tirely by  e8tin[iations  of  ear,  and  the  rule  much 
more  easy  to  manipulate  than  the  ordinary 
method  of  tuning. 


'Proof  of  Bute  for  Tuning  in  Equal  Temperament. 
Fob  Hasuoniuiis  and  Oboanb. 


f 


Notes 

e 

n 

271-2 
271-1 

274-0 
273-9 

277-2 
277-2 

286-5 

286-5 

d' 

287-4 
287-3 

290-3 
290-3 

2937 
293-6 

d% 

304-4 
304-3 

307-6 
307-4 

311-1 
3II-I 

322-6 
322-4 

325-9 
3259 

3296 
3296 

340-7 
3406 

/ 

341-7 
341-5 

345-3 
345-0 

349-2 
349-2 

360-9 
36x0 

ft 

g* 

n 

a' 

430-5 
430-4 

435-0 
435-0 

440-0 
440-0 

454-8 
454-7 

456-1 
455-9 

4609 
460-6 

466-2 
466-1 

481-8 
481-8 

V 

c* 

C'256 
Equal  . 
By  Bule 

256-0 
256-0 

258-7 
358-7 

261-6 
26x6 

362-0 
362-0 

365-8 
365-8 

370-0 
370-1 

383-6 
383-5 

387-6 
387-6 

392-0 
391*9 

406-4 
406-1 

410*6 
410-3 

415-3 
415-2 

4292 
4292 

483-3 
4831 

4883 

488-1 

493*9 
493-9 

510-4 
510-4 

512-0 
5120 

517-3 
517-3 

523*3 
5233 

540-8 
54D-8, 

Equal . 
By  Bule 

a'440 
Equal . 
By  Bule 

c'270-4 
Equal . 
By  Bule       . 

270-4 
270-4 

303-5 
303-5 

321-6 
321-5 

3824 
382-5 

405-2 
405-1 

Fob  Pianos. 


Notes 

/ 

170-9 
170-9 

180-5 
180-6 

i8i-o 
181-0 

191*2 
191-3 

9 

X9I-8 
191-8 

202-6 

202-6 

9Z 

2032 
2032 

214-6 
214-7 

a 

215-3 
215-2 

227-4 
2274 

air 

228-1 
2282 

240-9 

24I-I 

h 

241-7 

24X'7 

255-2 
255*4 

256 
256 

270-4 
270-4 

271-2 
271-2 

286*5 
2866 

d' 

287-4 

2873 

303*5 
3035 

d'l 

304-4 
304-6 

- 

321-6 

321-8 

322-6 
322-6 

340^7 
340-8 

341-7 
341-7 

361*0 
361-2 

Equal  . 
By  Bule 

Equal . 
By  Bule 

Hence  it  is  apparent  that  the  rule  ^ever 
makes  an  error  exceeding  -3  vib.,  and  generally 
keeps  below  this  limit.  Now  at  256  an  error 
of  -3  vib.  amounts  to  2  cents,  and  at  540  to 


less  than  1  cent.  The  rule,  therefore,  properly 
handled  will  give  results  equal  if  not  superior 
to  the  specimens  in  Art.  4. 


Digitized  by 


Google 


BSCT.  G. 


ON  TUNING  AND  INTONATION. 


491 


Art.  14. — The  rule  applies  only  to  one  octave  and  gives  what  are  known  as 
•  the  bearings,'  whence  the  other  notes  must  be  derived  by  taking  Octaves  in  the 
usual  way. 


Tuners  so  frequently  get  out  in  taking  the 
Octaves  that  it  is  convenient  to  have  a  (dieck 
on  the  estimation  of  ear.  This  is  furnished 
by  the  fact  that  any  note  will  beat  the  same 
number  of  times  in  a  second  with  an  imperfect 
Fourth  below  and  its  Octave  (that  is,  an  im- 
perfect Fifth)  above.  Thus  if  the  note  have 
401  vib.,  its  imperfect  Fourth  below  300,  and 
the  Octave  above  that  Fourth  below  600,  the 
beats  of  the  Fourth  are  3  x  401 -4  x  300=3, 
and  the  beats  of  the  Fifth  are  3  x  401  —  2  x  600 
=  3  also.  Now  the  imperfect  Fourths  are  fur- 
nished by  the  bearings  themselves.  Thus, 
going  upwards,  we  have  c'/  c",  c%  ft  c^ff ,  d* 
tf  d",  <fcc.  Going  downwards,  the  tuner  takes 
a  Fifth  and  then  a  Fourth,  as  h'  ef  h,Vbe^bh\), 
a'  d*  a,  and  so  on  from  octave  to  octave.  Mr. 
Hermann  Smith  prefers  to  insert  the  octaves 
above  when  tuning  the  original  bearings.  Thus, 
if  the  bearings  were  taken  in  the  two-foot  oc- 
tave d  c"  as  a'  xo  d'  15  gr*  loc/ 15/  15  a't  \od't 
15  fl^ff  lodt  15 /ff  15  fe'  10  e',  he  would  intro- 
duce the  octaves  in  this  order,  a  a*  d'  d"  ^  g 
d  d'f  fal  a!%  d'l  d"%  ^ff  g\  dl  d'%  fl  fl 
hU  d  d'  a\  But  the  method  I  have  proposed 
seems  simpler. 

The  principle  of  the  check  applies  to  the 
inversions  of  other  imperfect  intervals,  and 


may  serve  as  additional  verifications.  Thus 
a  note  beats  equally  with  an  imperfect  minor 
Sixth  below  and  its  Octave  the  imperfect  major 
Third  above.  Thus  500  :  801  beat  5  x  801 
-8x500  =  8,  and  801  :  1000  beat  5x801-4 
x  1000=8  also.  Again,  a  note  beats  equally 
with  an  imperfect  minor  Third  below  and  its 
Octave  the  major  Sixth  above.  Thus  500  :  601 
beat  5x601— 6x500  »  5,  and  601  :  1000  beat 
5  X  601  —3  X  1000=  5  also.  If  in  each  case  we 
inverted  tiie  order,  we  should  double  the  beats. 
Thus  for  the  Fifth,  200  :  301  will  beat  2  x  301 
—  3  X  200=  2 ;  but  301  :  400  will  beat  4  x  301  —  3 
X  400 = 4.  For  the  major  Third,  400  :  501  will  ^ 
beat  4  X  501  -  5  X  400  =  4 ;  but  501  :  800  will 
beat  8  X  501  —  5  X  800 = 8.  For  the  major  Sixth, 
300  :  501  will  beat  3  x  501  -  5  x  300  =  3 ;  but 
501  :  600  will  beat  6  x  501  —  5  x  600=6.  The 
reason  is  obvious.  The  fractions  expressing 
the  minor  intervals  f ,  f ,  f  have  odd  denomi- 
nators and  even  numerators,  and  hence  their 
inversions  reduce  by  dividing  by  2,  but  this  is 
not  the  case  for  the  major  intervals,  f ,  f ,  |. 

So  much  of  the  beauty  of  tuning  pianos, 
harmoniums,  and  organs  depends  on  the  per- 
fection of  the  Octave,  that  tuners  would  do 
wen  to  apply  the  first  test  with  the  Fourth 
below  and  Fifth  above,  as  a  matter  of  course. 


Art.  15. — Hule  for  tuning  in  meantone  temperament  from  d  2527,  HandeVs 
pitch,  to  &  283-6,  Father  Smith's  pitch  for  the  Durham  organ. 


As  will  be  seen  by  the  table  p.  4346,  c'264 
or  Helmholtz's  pitch  is  a  small  meantone 
Semitone,  and  the  Durham  pitch  is  a  mean- 
tone  Tone,  sharper  than  HandePs.  The  great 
flatness  of  the  Fifths  in  the  meantone  intona- 
tion makes  it  necessary  to  divide  ike  tunings 
into  three  classes,  sufficiently  ascertainable  by 
a  fork  in  Helmholt^'s  or  even  in  French  pitch. 
The  first  is  from  rather  less  than  a  Semitone 
to  about  a  Quartertone  flatter  than  French 
pitch ;  the  second  is  French  pitch  and  from  a 
Quartertone  flatter  to  a  Quartertone  sharper ; 


the  ^hird  is  from  a  Quartertone  to  a  Semitone  IT 
sharper  than  French. 

The  rule  would  extend  to  27  notes,  but  on 
the  proof  it  will  be  carried  out  only  for  14 
notes,  as  on  the  English  concertina,  for  which 
this  intonation  is  still  used.  And  for  this  in- 
strument the  '  tuning  octave '  may  be  taken  as 
d  to  d*.  For  the  few  organs  that  still  use  this 
intonation  the  same  Octave  must  be  tuned, 
and  hence,  when  taken  on  the  *  principal,'  the 
digitals  must  be  fingered  from  c  to  d,  because 
the  beats  would  be  otherwise  too  rapid  to  count 


Tune  in  the  following  order  the  numbers  25-6-7  and  40-1-2,  meaning  that 
the  beats  are  to  be  25  and  40  for  the  low,  26  and  41  for  the  medium,  and  27  and  42 
for  the  high  pitch  in  10  sec.,  according  to  the  three  grades  already  laid  down. 

c'  25-6-7  g'  40-1-2  d'  25-6-7  a'  40-1-2  e'  25-6-7  b'  40-1-2 /J  40-1-2  c'J  25-6-7 
^'#40-1-2.  If 

d  40-1-2/  40-1-2  6't>  25-6-7  e'\}  40-1-2  a]}. 

The  tuning  is  conducted  in  the  same  way  2^    • 

as  before,  only  in  two  series,  from  d  to  g'tt  and  4d' 

from  d  to  a'b,  making  the  Fifths  and  Fourths  2a' 

at  first  perfect,  and  then  the  Fifths  closer  and  4d    > 

the  Fourths  wider.    But  in  two  cases  df^fb^b  26'    -• 

the  Fourth  is  taken  upwards,  and  then  the  4/^9 

upper  note  has  to  be  sharpened.    And  there  is  4di  ■■ 

an  additional  verification  after  tuning  to  e',  for  2/5  ■■ 

the  major  Thirds  cV,  gf'6',  d'ft ,  a'dZ  ,  difZ  ,  Ad'Z 

and  also  fa'  and  minor  Sixths  d'b'b^  da'b  

should  be  all  sensibly  perfect.    Octaves  can  3/    • 

be  verified  by  imperfect  Fourths  more  easily  3yb  = 

than  in  equal  temperament.  3e'b 

The  equations  for  determining  the  pitch  3a' b  = 
irom  the  beats  are 


'3c'  -2-5.-6-7 

-39'  -4'o--i-*2 

.3d'  -2-5.-6-7 

»3a'  —  40--X--2 

■•3d  -2-5--6-7 

=  36      —  4-0-' I -'2 

-3f9  -4*o-'i-'2 
=  3c'ff  -2-S--6-7 
■'Ztft  -4*o--i--2 

•4c'  +4*o--i-'2 
=  4/*  +4-0--I--2 
-.2b'b  +2-5--6-7 
■4db  +4-0--I--2 


Digitized  by  VjOOQIC 


492 


ADDITIONS  BY  THE  TRANSLATOR. 


AFP.  XX« 


'Proof  of  the  Bulefor  Tuning  in  Meantone  Temperament. 


Notes 


Handel  . 
By  Bole  . 


2527 
2527 


Helmholtz 
By  Rule  . 


Durham . 
By  Rule  . 


264*0 
264-0 


283-6 
283-6 


264-1 
264-1 


d'        d'l 


282-5  295-2 
282-41295-I 


302-8 
302 


275-9  295-2^308-4 
27S*8|295-o  308-3 


296-3]3i7-i 
296-6317-0 


331-3 
331-6 


^b 


315-9 
9 


431S 


315-8 
3159 


339-2 
339-2 


338-0 
3383 


330-0 
329-9 


354-5 
354-6 


353-2 
353-4 


353-1 
353-4 


379-2 
379-5 


ft 


369-0 
3691 


377-9394-9404-3 
377-8        ^ 


394-8 
394*7 


3963 
396-9424-1 


g't 


a'b 


_.  ._  422-5 

394-8  4046  422-5 


b'b 


412-5 
412-5 


424-0443-1 


443-5 


453-7 
453-7 


422-4441-4 
422-6441*2 


472-3 
472- 


474-1 
474-2 


472-4505-4 
505-4 


452-1 
452-3472-5 


_  493-5 
6493*6 


507-3 
507-4 


530-1 


530-6567 


528-0 
528-0 


5672 
2 


The  results  are  seen  to  be  nearly  as  good 
OB  before,  only  the  Durham  ft  is  396*9  in 


place  of  396-3,  an  error  of  *6  vib.  and  2*6  oents, 
which  is  scarcely  perceptible. 


Art.  16. — ^In  the  Froceedings  of  the  Mibsical  Association  for  1874-5,  vol.  L 
^  pp.  141-145,  Mr.  Bosanquet  gives  the  process  that  he  followed  in  toning  his 
cycle  of  53  (see  sect.  F.,  p.  479),  but  it  is  too  complicated  to  be  abstracted  with 
a  reasonable  hope  of  its  being  understood.  It  depended  mainly  on  taking  the 
beats  of  the  differential  tones  in  the  major  chord,  with  the  major  Third  either 
in  the  middle  or  highest.  In  this  case  the  Fifth  was  presumed  to  be  accurately 
tuned  to  begin  with.  Taking  the  numbers  in  sect.  A.  art.  27,  but  doubling  them 
for  the  Octave  higher  and  supposing  the  Fourth  to  be  perfect,  we  have 

c"528,  /'704,  a"879'32  in  the  cycle. 
Differentials  176       175*32 

Beats  per  second  *68    ,  that  is,  beats  per  minute  40*8 

and  the  beats  were  apparently  counted  for  a  minute.  Mr.  Bosanquet,  however, 
does  not  recommend  the  process  for  his  harmonium  because  the  differential  tones 
were  not  distinct  enough. 

Art.  17. — Mr.  Paul  White  has  two  methods  of  tuning  the  cycle  of  53  by  means 
^  of  beats.  We  may  suppose  that  for  the  given  pitch  of  the  initial  c,  aJl  the  pitch 
numbers  have  been  calculated,  as  in  sect.  A,  art.  27,  for  No.  4,  Mr.  Bosanquet's 
initial  0=264  vib.  Two  places  of  decimals  are  required  for  ijiis  purpose,  as  in 
both  methods  it  is  necessary  to  rely  upon  the  slow  beats  of  the  Thirds  and  Sixths 
and  check  the  result  at  every  few  steps. 

First  method.  Tune  5  minor  Thirds  of  the  cycle  up,  alternating  with  major 
Sixths  down,  to  keep  within  the  same  octave.  Since  a  minor  Third  has  14  degrees 
this  gives  5  x^=i^,  or  an  Octave  and  17  degrees,  that  is  an  Octave  above  the 
major  Third  of  the  cycle,  which  will  beat  slowly  with  the  note  on  which  we 
started.  Thus  beginning  with  264  and  taking  first  3  cyclic  minor  Thirds  up,  and 
then  a  cyclic  major  Bixth  down,  followed  by  a  cyclic  minor  Third  up,  we  have 


vib 264 

beats  in  70  sec. 


np  np  up  down  np 

317*05  380-75  457'25  274-56  329-73 

12  14-5  17-5  io'5  12-9 


and  as  264  :  329*73  is  a  cyclic  and  not  a  just  msgor  Third,  which  would  be  264  :  330, 
^  it  will  beat  10*8  times  in  10  seconds,  and  this  will  be  the  verification  of  the  work. 
Observe  that  the  interval  of  the  minor  Third  must  be  too  wide,  and  hence  of  the 
major  Sixth  too  close,  so  that  when  we  tune  up,  the  uppemoi&  must  be  made  sharp, 
and  when  we  tune  down,  the  lower  note  must  be  made  sharp.  Also  that  as  the 
5th  and  6th  partials  are  involved  in  the  beats,  the  method  will  suit  only  qualities 
of  tone,  like  reed-tones,  with  strong  upper  partials.  Observe  also  that  in  equal 
temperament  5  equal  minor  Thirds  are  an  Octave  and  an  equal  minor  (not  a  major) 
Third.  Having  completed  one  set  proceed  with  the  next  set  of  5  minor  Thirds  (or 
major  Sixths)  until  the  whole  cycle  is  complete  for  one  octave  and  then  tune  by 
Octaves. 

Second  method,  which  Mr.  Paul  White  prefers.  Tune  7  cyclic  major  Thirds 
down  (alternating  with  minor  Sixths  up  to  keep  within  the  same  octave).  The 
result  will  be  a  cyclic  Pythagorean  minor  Third  of  13  degrees  down,  or  40  degrees 
up,  for  3-7xH=3-W=3-2-if=i-|J=f5.  And  this  can  be  verified  by 
3  cyclic  Fifths  up,  for  3  x  f  J=ff =1  J§,  such  Fifths  being  practically  perfect.  Thus 
beginning  at  528  vib.  we  obtain,  taking  major  Thirds  down,  and  minor  Sixths  up : 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQl€ 


«BCT.  It.     THE  HISTORY  OF  MUSICAL  PITCH  IN  EUROPE.  493 

down  down  down  np  down  down  up 

^b 528     42274     338-47     271-00     433*95     347'44     278-18    445*45 

beatsin  losec.  17  13-9         11-2         17-5  14  11-4         17-9 

a  Fourth  down       another  Fourth  down  a  Fifth  up 

^b 528       396  297  445-5 

Ihe  Fourths  and  Fifths  are  taken  just,  and  the>  result  agrees  to  -05  vib.  It  must 
he  remembered  that  the  cyclic  major  Thirds  are  too  close,  hence  in  tuning  down 
the  lower  note  must  be  sharpened.  On  the  contrary  the  cyclic  minor  Sixths  will 
be  too  wide,  and  hence  in  tuning  up,  the  upper  note  has  to  be  sharpened.  Having 
completed  this  set  of  7  proceed  to  another,  till  the  cycle  is  complete.  This  method 
also  only  suits  qualities  of  tone,  like  reed-tones,  with  powerful  5th  and  8th  partials. 

The  process  thus  carried  out  would  of  course  be  tedious,  and  Mr.  Paul  White 
seems  to  assume  a  tolerably  uniform  beat,  perhaps  of  15  in  10  seconds,  for  he  says: 
'  The  beats  cannot  of  course  be  made,  or  be  made  to  remain  uniform,  but  if  they 
are  nearly  so,  or  if  a  few  do  not  beat  at  all,  the  temperament  is  still  good.  1 1| 
have  found  that  the  Fifbhs  can  be  kept  almost  entirely  free  from  beats  by  talong  good 
care  of  the  very  slow  beats  of  the  Thirds.  I  have  long  been  convinced  that  beats 
in  the  middle  octave  do  much  more  good  than  harm  in  a  musical  cycle,  for  it  would 
be  impossible  to  tune  a  musical  cycle  of  any  size  correctly  without  them.  The  least 
scratch  on  a  reed  will  change  a  beat,  while  it  often  takes  quite  a  scrape  to  cause 
a  beat  where  none  existed.'  The  processes  Mr.  Paul  White  has  worked  out  with 
the  ingenious  system  of  checks,  shew  that  he  is  a  thorough  master  of  the  whole 
art  of  tuning,  and,  a  rare  thing  to  be  met  with  among  professional  tuners  or  even 
musicians,  perfectly  understands  its  rationale. 

Art.  18. — A  succession  of  just  Fifths,  as  mentioned  in  art.  i,  is  very  difficult  to 
tune  ;  and  one  of  just  major  Thirds  is  still  more  difficult.  Hence  an  auxiliary  stop 
on  an  organ  or  an  auxiliary  harmonium  is  required  when  just  intervals  have  to  be 
tuned. 

It  is  not  difficult  to  ascertain  by  ear  whether  tuned  i  beat  in  a  second  sharper  than  the 

a  Fifth  or  major  Third  is  considerably  too  flat,  auxiliary  d\    And  in  this  way  by  a  laborious  m 

Suppose  we  start  with  c',  then  tune  an  auxiliary  double  process  the  succession  of  Fifths  could 

g'  (indicated  by  a  roman  letter)  decidedly  flat  be  tuned  with  great  accuracy.    For  the  major 

beating  40  times  in  10  seconds  with  cf.    Then  Thirds,  tune  an  auxiliary  e'  decidedly  flat,  and 

3c' — 2g' «  4,  so  that  |c'  =  g'  +  2 ,  but  fc'  is  the  beating  4  in  a  second  with  c'.  Then  $(/  -  40'  ==  4, 

perfect  Fifth  to  </,  hence  we  must  tune  the  re-  and  true  e/Bfc'»e'+ 1.    In  the  same  way  we 

quired  Fifth  9' »  g'  +  2,  that  is,  sharper  than  g'  could  get  g2Jt  and  b^'t »    But  for  a' b,  /'b,  ^'b b 

by  2  beats  in  a  second.    For  the  next  Fifth  in  we  must  tune  auxiliary  minor  Sixths,  which  is 

order  to  remain  in  the  same  octave  we  should  troublesome  and  not  feasible  except  on  reed 

take  the  Fourth  down.    Tune  the  auxiliary  d'  instruments.    Tune  an  auxiliary  a'b  flat,  so  as 

so  that  it  should  be  too  flat,  and  beat  4  times  to  beat  5  times  in  a  second  with  &.    Then 

in  a  second  with  the  correct  &.    Then  3^  8c'— 5a'b  =  5,and  truea"b«Sc'  =  a'6+ 1.    And 

—  4d'  =  4,  and  f^ = d'  +  i .    But  Jgr  is  the  correct  so  on. 
d',  or  Fourth  below  ^.    Hence  it  must  be 

It  appears,  then,  that  tempered  intervals  which  present  beats  of  their  own  are 
more  easy  to  tune  than  just  intervals  for  which  an  auxiliary  beating  tone  has  to  be 
supplied.  The  only  satisfactory  way,  however,  of  tuning  perfect  and  tempered 
intervals  is  by  a  fork  tonometer,  one  of  which  suffices  for  every  possible  case  that 
can  arise,  when  once  the  pitch  numbers  of  the  notes  have  been  calculated  as  in  f 
sect.  A. 

SECTION  H. 

THE   HIBTORT  OF  ITDSZCAL  PITCH  IN  EUROPE, 

(See  note  p.  16.) 

Art.  Table  I. — continued, 

1.  Pitch  of  a  Note,  p.  494.  4.  Mean  Pitch  of  Europe  for  Two  Cen- 

2.  Musical  Pitch,  p.  494.  turies,  p.  495. 

3.  Early  Pitch,  p.  494.  5.  The  Compromise  Pitch,  p.  497. 

4.  Materials  and  Authorities,  p.  494.  6.  Modem      Orchestral      Pitch,      and 

5.  Description  of  the  Tables,  p.  494.  ♦Church  Pitch  Medium,  p.  499. 
Table  I.  Historical  Pitches  in  order  from  the  7.  Church  Pitch,  high,  p.  503. 

Lowest  to  the  Highest,  p.  495-  8.  Church  Pitch,  highest,  p.  503. 

1.  Church  Pitch,  lowest,  p.  495.  9.  Chamber  Pitch,  highest,  and  Church 

2.  Church  Pitch,  low,  p.  495.  Pitch,  extreme,  p.  504. 

3.  Chamber  Pitch,  low,  p.  495. 


Digitized  by  VjOOQlC 


494 


ADDITIONS  BY  THE   TRANSLATOR. 


APP.  XX. 


Table  II.  Classified    Index    to    Table    I., 
p.  504. 

I.  Austro-Hungary,  p.  504. 
II.  Belgium,  p.  504. 

III.  England,   Scotland,  and    Ireland, 

p.  505- 

IV.  France,  p.  508. 
V.  Germany,  p.  509. 

VI.  Holland,  p.  510. 
VII.  Italy,  p.  510. 
VIII.  Russia,  p.  510. 
IX.  Spain,  p.  511. 

X.  United  States  of  America,  p.  511. 
GoMGLnsioNS,  p.  511. 
Art. 
6.  The  History  of  Musical  Pitch  in  Europe 
for  500  years,  p.  511. 
^    7.  Original    Motives    for    determination   of 
Church  Pitch,  p.  511. 


Art. 
8. 


Effect  of  the  foot  measure  of  different 
countries  on  the  pitch  of  organs,  p.  511. 

Origin  of  Chamber  Pitch,  p.  512. 

Evolution  of  the  Mean  Pitch,  its  great 
extent  geographically  and  chronologi- 
cally, p.  512. 

Difficulties  arising  from  singing  at  hi^ 
pitch  classical  music  written  for  mean 
pitch,  p.  512. 

How  the  rise  in  pitch  commenced  and 
spread,  p.  512. 

The  Compromise  Pitch  in  France,  England, 
and  Germany,  p.  512. 

14.  Variations  in  English  Organ  Pitch,  p.  513. 

1 5.  Rise  in  pitch  connected  with  wind  instru- 

ments, p.  513. 

16.  What  must  be  done,  p.  513. 


12. 


13 


Art.  I. — The  pitch  number  of  a  note  has  been  already  defined  as  the  number  of 
double  vibrations  which  the  sonoroos  body  producing  it  makes  and  commnmcates 
in  one  second  (p.  iia). 

Art.  2. — The  pitch  number  of  a  musical  instrument,  or  briefly  its  musiccd 
pitch,  is  taken  to  be  the  pitch  number  of  the  tuning  note  at  a  temperature  of 
59°F.=i5°C.=i2°R. 


The  tuning  note  is  here  assumed  to  be  the 
a'  of  the  violin,  from  which  the  pitch  number 
of  all  the  other  notes  in  the  scale  must  be  cal- 
culated, or  determined  approximately  by  ear 
from  the  temperament  (sect.  A.)  and  system 


of  tuning  (sect.  G.)  in  use.  By  taking  a'  as 
the  tuning  note,  the  inquiry  is  practically 
limited  to  European  music  within  the  last  500 
years. 


Art.  3. — The  following  passage  from  Syntagmatis  musid,  Michaelis  Pr^tobh 
C,  Tomus  Secundus,  de  Organographia,  1619,  p.  14,  explains  the  condition  of 
^  early  pitch. 


*  In  the  first  place  it  must  be  known  that 
the  pitch,  both  of  organs  and  other  musical 
instruments,  varies  greatly.  Since  the  ancients 
were  not  accustomed  to  play  in  concerfc  with 
all  kinds  of  instruments  at  the  same  time, 
.wind  instruments  were  very  differently  made 
and  intoned  by  instrument  makers,  some  high 
and  some  low.  For  the  higher  an  instrument 
is  intoned  in  its  own  kind  and  manner,  as 


trumpets,  shawms,  and  treble  viols,  the  more 
freshly  it  sounds  and  resounds.  On  the  con- 
trary, the  deeper  trombones,  bassoons,  bassa- 
neldi,  bombards,  and  bass  viols  are  tuned,  the 
more  majestic  and  magnificent  is  their  stately 
march.  Hence  when  the  organs,  positives, 
olavioymbals,  and  other  wind  instruments  are 
not  in  the  same  pitch  with  each  other  the 
musician  is  much  plagued.' 


Art.  4. — The  authorities  on  whom  1  rely  are  minutely  specified  in  my  *  History 
of  Musical  Pitch '  in  the  Journal  of  the  Society  of  Arts  for  5  March  and  2  April 
1880,  and  7  Jan.  1881.  The  two  last  papers  contain  indispensable  corrections 
and  additions.  Li  the  privately  printed  copies  there  was  an  addendum  on  U.S. 
America  from  Messrs.  C.  K.  Cross  and  W.  T.  Miller,  American  Journal  of  Otology, 
Oct.  1880. 


f  Here  it  must  suffice  to  say  that  after  learn- 
ing to  determine  pitch  to  ^  vib.  (p.  444)  I 
obtained  the  loan  of  authentic  forks  from  the 
Society  of  Arts,  Mr.  A.  J.  Hipkins,  Rev.  G.  T. 
DriflSeld  (Handel's),  Fran  Naeke  of  Dresden, 
Prof.  Rossetti  of  Padua,  Mr.  Blaikley,  and  Dr. 
W.  H.  Stone.  I  procured  compared  copies  of 
forks  in  the  Conservatoire  at  Paris,  and  others 
tuned  at  known  temperatures  to  remarkable 
organs  at  Vienna,  Dresden,  Hamburg,  Stras- 
burg,  and  Seville.  Then,  with  the  assistance 
of  many  organists,  I  measured  numerous 
organs  in  England  of  which  the  pitch  had  not 
been  changed,  or,  with  the  kind  help  of  Beveral 
organ-builders,  obtained  untouched  pipes  of 
altered  organs.  When  these  failed  I  had 
models  made  of  pipes  of  which  the  dimensions 
were  given  by  Schlick  1511,  PrsRtorius  1619, 
Mersenne    1636,  Tomkins    1668,  Bi'-dos    1766, 


and  others,  which  were  obligingly  presented  to 
me  by  Mr.  T.  Hill,  the  organ -builder,  on  whose 
bellows  I  measured  them.  These  constituted 
my  own  materials.  Then  I  had  recourse  to 
the  measurements  and  lists  of  Cagniard  de  la 
Tour,  CavailU-Goll,  de  la  Fage,  Delezenne,  de 
Prony,  Euler,  Fischer,  French  Commission  on 
Pitch,  Eoenig,  Lissajous,  McLeod,  Marpuig, 
Naeke,  Sauveur,  Scheibler,  Schmahl,  Dr.  R. 
Smith,  and  others.  From  these  I  constructed 
the  lists  which  follow.  In  my  original  papers 
each  pitch  is  accompanied  with  full  details. 
Here  I  give  the  smallest  possible  account. 

Art..  5. — The  pitch  given  is  always  that  of 
a',  where  possible  at  59°  F.  But  this  was  not 
always  the  note  measured.  When  it  was  not, 
a'  was  calculated  on  the  assumption  of  either 
meantone  or  equal  temperament.  Assuming 
a  lowest  ideal  pitch  of  a'370,  which  has  never 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQIC 


SECT.  H.     THE   HISTOEY  OF  MUSICAL  PITCH  IN   EUEOPE. 


495 


yet  been  found,  I  g^ve  the  cents  by  which  any 
other  pitch  exceeds  this,  so  that  the  interviJ 
between  any  two  pitches  is  immediately  deter- 
mined by  subtracting  the  cents.  I  give  also 
the  date,  adding  occasionally  a  for  ante^  before, 
;p  for  2)05^  after,  and  c  for  circa^  about ;  and 
the  authority,  or  observer,  where  £.  means 
that  I  am  responsible  for  the  measurement, 
directly  or  indirectly.    Finally,  I  add  a  list, 


classified  by  countries,  stating  the  kind  of 
pitch.  I  hav^  not  thought  it  necessary  to  give 
absolutely  every  fork  and  pitch  entered  in  my 
*  History,*  but  have  reported  a  large  number  of 
these  entries,  and  especially  all  the  most  inter- 
esting of  them.  A  complete  Grerman  trans- 
lation of  my  paper  is  in  preparation,  and  will 
be  published  at  Vienna. 


Table  I. — Historical  Pitches  in  obdeb  fbom  Lowest  to  Highest. 


Cents  I      a'      |    Date 


Obeerrer 


Place  and  other  particalars 


370 

— 

373-1 

— 

3737 

1648 

374-2 

I7c»a 

376-6 

1766 

377 

1511 

384-3 
3846 
3922 

1 700c 

1851 

1739 

393-2 
395-2 

17x3 
1759 

395-8 

1720 
1789 

396-4 

161S 

3987 

1854a 

199     415       "754 


I.  Church  Fitch,  Lowest 


E. 
Delezenne 

B. 

Delezenne 

E. 

E. 


4029 

1648 

403-9 

1730 

406-6 
407-3 
407-9 

1704 
1854 
1762 

409 

1783 

410 

— 

41 1-4 

1688 

4133 
414-4 

1776 

Ideal  lowest  pitch  or  zero  point 

Calculated  from  D.'s  measurements  of  an  open 

wooden  pipe  1-3  metres  long,  taken  as  c 
Paris,  from  a  model  after  Mersenne 
Lille,  organ  of  I'Hospice  Comtesse 
Paris,  from  a  model  after  B§dos 
Heidelberg,  from  a  model  after  Arnold  Schlick 

(see  535  cents) 


2.  Church  Pitch f  Low: 


Delezenne     . 

Euler   . 

Stockhausen  &  E. 
Dr.  B.  Smith 


McLeod  &  E. 

E. 
Delezenne    • 


Lille,  old  fork  found  1854a  by  M.  Mazingue 
Lille,  organ  of  St.  Sauveur,  rebuilt,  with  old  pitch 
St.  Petersburg,  a  clavichord  according  to  Marpurg, 

but  Euler  gives  no  particulars 
Strassburg  Minster,  great  organ  by  A.  Silbermann 
Cambridge,  Bernhardt  Schmidt's  organ  at  Trinity 
College,   1708,    after    being    new    voiced    and 
*  shifted'  in  1759 
Bome,  pitch-pipes  observed  by  Dr.  B.  Smith 
France,  Versailles,  copy  of  fork  No.  410  at  the 
MuB^e  du  Conservatoire,  Paris,  compared  with 
the  original  by  Cavaill6-.Coll 
Palatinate  of  the  Bhine,  from  a  model  of  pipe  de- 
scribed by  Salomon  de  Cans 
Lille,  old  organ  of  La  Madeleine  restored 


3.  Chamber  Pitch^  Low. 


E.         .       . 

E. 

Sauveur 
Delezenne    . 
Schmahl  &  E. 


Lissajous 


Schmahl  &  E. 

Naeke  . 
Marpurg 


Paris,  Mersenne's  Spinet,  from  his  statement  that 
Bq  =  B^dos's  4-foot  c  (see  31  cents) 

Padua,  from  copy  sent  by  Prof.  Bossetti  of  the  old 
lower  /'  fork  of  the  bellf oundry  of  Colbacchini 

Paris,  result  of  several  experiments  on  an  e  pipe 

Lille,  organ  of  St.  Maurice  repaired,  old  pitch  kept 

Hamburf?,  organ  of  St.  Michaelis  Kirche,  buUt 
by  Hildebrand  of  Dresden,  imder  the  direction 
of  Handel's  friend,  J.  Mattheson  (1681-1764), 
in  the  chamber  pitch  of  the  period,  still  pre- 
served; now,  and  probably  always,  in  equal 
temperament 

Paris,  Court  clavecins,  fork  of  Pascal  Taskin, 
their  tuner 

Paris,  1 8th  century  pitch-pipe  found  in  the  cabinet 
of  the  Faculty  of  Sciences 

EEamburg,  chamber  pitch  on  the  former  8-foot 
Qedact  of  the  St.  Jacobi  organ  (see  484  cents) 

*  Schneider's  Oboe,'  date  and  place  unknown 

Breslau,  clavichords 


4.  Mean  Pitch  of  Ewropc  for  Two  Centuries, 
E. 


IT 


IT 


Dresden,  organ  of  the  Boman  Catholic  Church  by 
Gottfried  Silbermann,  pitch  of  the  chained  fork 
placed  there  by  King  August  der  Orechte, 
1 763-1827,  who  would  not  allow  the  pitch  to  be 
changed ;  the  fork  was  lent  me  by  Frau  Naeke 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


496  ADDITIONS  BY  THE  TRANSLATOB.  app.xx. 

Tablb  I. — Historical  Pijcrkb  in  obdiek  fbom  Lowest  to  Hiobist — ecmiinued. 


f 


IT 


Oenta 


Date 


Obeerrer 


Place  and  other  particulan 


20I    1 

415-5 

211 
212 

418 
4181 

215 

419 

217 
218 

4»9*5 
4196 

»» 

II 

219 

4199 

220 

420-1 

224 

421-2 

225 

421-3 

226 

421-6 

229 

422-3 

>i 

II 

230 

422-5 

*i 

II 

»i 

II 

II 

II 

II 

422*6 

II 

II 

II 

II 

231 

422-7 

23* 

423 

233 

4232 

1} 

II 

II 

4233 

4.  Mean  Pitch 
1722     Naeke  . 


1780 
1878 

1 700c 

1714 
1858 
1785 


Euler   . 
E. 

E. 

Naeke  . 
de  la  Fage 
E. 


1715c  E. 


1780 

i860 

1780 

1780 

1780 
17800 

1751 


1820a 


E. 

E. 

Naeke  . 

Naeke  &  E 

Naeke  . 
E. 

E. 


E. 


1838 

E. 

I877P 

E.         .        . 

1790a 

E.         .        . 

1754c 

Delezenne    . 

1 780c 

E. 

18000 

E.         .        . 

1820 

MoLeod  &  E. 

1778 

E. 

1815- 
1821 

E. 

1813 

E. 

of  Europe  for  Two  Centuries — oontinaed. 

Dresden,  organ  of  St.  Sophie,  built  by  G.  Silber- 
mann 

St.  Petersburg,  organs ;  no  particnlars 

Dresden,  present  pitch  of  the  organ  of  the  Roman 
Catholic  Church,  from  a  fork  tuned  for  me  there 

London,  Benatas  Harris's  organ  at  St.  John's, 
Clerkenwell 

Freiberg,  Saxony,  G.  Silbermann's  organ 

Madrid,  ton  de  ohapelle,  calculated 

Seville,  Spain,  pitch  of  the  old  organ  of  Torje 
Bosch,  from  a  fork  said  by  the  organist  Don 
Yniguez  to  be  in  exact  unison  with  its  a'  at  a 
mean  temperature 

England,  rude  tenor  a  fork,  belonging  to  Rev.  G.  T. 
Driffield,  who  held  it  to  have  been  made  by  John 
Shore,  the  inventor  of  tuning-forks 

Winchester  College  organ,  from  one  of  the  pipes 
added  by  Green  when  repairing  B.  Harris's 
organ  of  168 1 

Bussian  Imperial  Court  church  band  from  fork 
lent  by  Frau  Naeke 

Vienna,  fork  of  the  Saxon  organ-builder  Schulze, 
who  lived  at  Vienna  in  Mozart's  time 

Vienna,  copy  of  fork  of  Stein,  who  made  Mozart's 
clavichords  and  pianos,  lent  me  by  Frau  Naeke 

Dresden,  fork  of  former  Court  organist  Kirsten 

Verona,  from  a  copy  of  a  c'  fork  believed  to  be  the 
Boman  pitch  of  1780,  preserved  at  the  bell- 
foundry  of  Cavedini,  procured  by  Prof.  Bossetti 
of  Padua 

England,  Handel's  fork  belonging  to  Bev.  G.  T. 
Driffield.  The  organ  at  Cannons  in  the  private 
chapel  of  the  Duke  of  Chandos,  bmlt  by  Jordans, 
and  afterwards  bought  by  Trinity  Church,  Gos- 
port,  has  been  recently  (in  1884)  examined  by 
the  organist,  Mr.  Hewlett,  and  found  to  have 
had  in  Handel's  time,  when  he  used  to  play  on 
it,  a  Bq  (now  Bb)  pipe  of  12-3  inches  long, 
and  I  inch  in  diameter ;  this  shews  that  its  pit<^ 
was  then  ^1423-5,  or  practically  the  same  as 
Handel's  fork   . 

Westminster  Abbey,  as  originally  tuned  by 
Schreider  and  Jordans,  from  indications  by  Mr. 
T.  Hill,  who  retuned  it  to  a'44i*7.  It  had  been 
altered  by  Greatorex  to  0^433*2,  Smart's  pitch 

Bath  Abbey  Church,  as  rebuilt  by  Smith  of  Bristol, 
from  indications  by  Mr.  T.  HUl 

England,  Mr.  J.  Curwen's  Tonic  Solfa  standard 
c''507,  using  the  just  a'  only 

Eew  Parish  Church,  Green's  organ,  untouched 
and  in  meantone  temperament  when  measived 
in  1878,  built  as  a  ohunber  organ  for  George  IIL 

Lille,  very  old  fork  found  in  workshops  of  M. 
FranQois,  musical  instrument  maker  there 

Padua,  from  copy  of  the  higher  /'  fork  of  the  bell- 
foundry  of  Colbacohini  (see  152  cents) 

England,  from  old  fork,  c"505-7,  belonging  to 
Messrs.  Broadwoods 

Paris,  Th64tre  Feydeau,  Op^a  Comique,  from  copy 
of  fork  at  the  Conservatoire,  Paris,  compared 
with  the  original  by  Cavaill6-Coll 

London,  Green's  organ  at  St.  Katharine's,  Begent's 
Park,  still  (when  I  measured  it)  in  meantone 
temperament  (see  sect  G.,  p.  484CO 

Dresden,  band  of  the  opera  while  C.  M.  von 
Weber  (i  786.1826)  was  conductor  {KdpM- 
meister) 

London,   second  copy  of    Peppercorn's  fori:  by 
which   the   pianofortes  of    the   Philharmonic 
Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


6CT.  H.     THE  HISTORY  OP  MUSICAL  PITCH  IN  EUROPE.  497 

Table  I. — Historical  Pitches  in  order  frou  Lowest  to  Highest — continued. 


Date 


Observer 


Place  and  other  particulaia 


4. 

4237 

I8I3 

4241 

1740- 
I8I2 

4242 

I6I9 

l» 

1823 

424-3 

1750a 

II 

1749 

424-4 

1833 

4246 

I800C 

424-9 

1805 

249 

250 
250 

251 


255 


260 

262 


425-2 

425-5 
4256 

425-8 


425-9 

426-5 

427 

4272 

427-5 
427-6 
4277 


4278 
428-7 


430 
430-4 


1 800c 

1 730c 
.I780C 

1829 

1740- 

1780 
1764 

1824 


1839 


I8IOC 
I70I 


Mean  Pitch  of  Europe  for  Two  Centuries — continued. 

Society  were  originally  tuned;  this  copy  was 
prepared  for  the  Society  of  Arts  in  i860,  and  is 
now  in  the  possession  of  Messrs.  Broadwoods 
London,  first  copy  of  Peppercorn's  fork  made  be- 
fore i860,  belonging  to  Mr.  Hlpkins;  see  last 
entry,  the  original  is  lost,  and  it  is  impossible  to 
say  which  was  correct.  The  difference,  2  cents, 
is  utterly  insignificant 

Naeke  .  .  .  Eutin  (18  miles  N.  of  Liibeck),  fork  of  Franz 
Anton  von  Weber,  father  of  Carl  Maria  von 
Weber 

E.         .        .        .     Brunswick,  from  a  model  made  from  Prastorius's 

drawing  of  an  organ  pipe  at  a  *  suitable  *  church    % 
pitch 

Blscher  .     Paris,  Italian  Opera,  mean  of  twenty  measure- 

ments of  a  fork  given  by  Spontini 

E.  .  .  .  London,  old  forks  formerly  belonging  to  Prof. 
Faraday,  lent  me  by  Mr.  D.  J.  Blaikley 

E.  .  .  .  London,  organ  at  All  Hallows  the  Great  and  Less, 
Upper  Thames  Street,  built  by  Glyn  &  Parker, 
by  whom  Handel's  Foundling  Hospital  organ 
was  built 

E.  .  .  .  Weimar,  from  a  model  of  Tdpfer's  wide  principal 
c"-pipe 

E.  .        .     England,  old  fork  said  to  have  been  used  in  Ply- 

mouth Theatre,  lent  me  by  Dr.  Stainer 

E.  ' .  .  .  London,  old  D  fork  of  Elliott's,  by  which  he  tuned 
the  organ  built  for  the  Ancient  Concerts  at  the 
Hanover  Square  Booms,  lent  me  by  his  suc- 
cessor, Mr.  T.  Hill 

Naeke  .        .        .     Germany,  fork  of  the  bassoonist  Eummer 

E.  .  .  .  Padua,  mean  of  two  ancient  pitch-pipes  belonging 
to  the  bellf oundry  of  Colbacchini,  lent  me  at 
the  request  of  Prof.  F.  Bossetti  there 

Lissajous      .  Paris,  pitch  of  opera  piano  as  distinct  from  the 

orchestra,  verified  by  Monneron  for  de  la  Fage 

E.  .  .  .  England,  Schnetzler's  organ  at  the  German  Chapel 
Boyal,  St.  James's  Palace 

E.  .  .  .  Halifax,  Schnetzler's  organ,  from  indications  by 
Mr.  T.  Hill 

Lissajous  .  .  Paris,  pitch  of  opera,  suddenly  lowered  on  31 
March  for  Mme.  Branchu,  whose  voice  was  fail- 
ing. The  piano  for  rehearsals  was  also  lowered, 
and  was  not  raised  inmiediately  when  the  or- 
chestra was  raised ;  this  was  called  opera  pitch 

de  la  Fage    .        .     Bologna,  Italy,  pitch  of  fork  of  Tadolini,  the  best 
tuner  in  the  town 
Great  Yarmouth,  St.  George's  Chapel,  by  Byfield, 

Jordan  &  Bridge 
Wimbledon  Church,  organ  built  by  Messrs.  Walker    at 
Paris,  Grand  Opera 
Norwich  Cathedral  organ  before  it  was  altered  by 

Bryceson,  supposed  to  be  by  B.  Harris 
Tonic  Solfa  pitch  to  1877,  afterwards  422-5 
Paris,  Th6dtre  Feydeau,  fork  given  by  Spontini 
London,  old  organ  built  by  B.  Harris,  a  pipe  of  St. 
Andrew  Undershaft,  from  Green's  organ,  pre- 
served by  Mr.  T.  Hill 

E.  .  .  .  St.  George's  Chapel,  Windsor,  measured  in  Feb. 
1880,  while  stiU  in  meantone  temperament 

Ions  .        .     Newcastle-on-Tyne,   St.  Nicholas   Church  organ 

built  by  Benatus  Harris,  frequently  altered  ex- 
cept in  pitch 

5.  The  Comjpromiee  Pitch. 

Lissajous      .        .     Paris,  fork  of  M.  Lemoine,  a  celebrated  amateur 
E.         .        .        .     Fulliam  Parish  Church  organ,  built  by  Jordans. 
This  pitch  was  officially  adopted  in  Italy  in  1884 

K  K  T 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQ IC 


1740  ;  Tunbridge  &  E. 


E. 

Scheibler 

E. 


1843 
1811 
1878a 

1877a  E. 
1823  '  Fischer 
1696  E. 


1788 
1670 


498  ADDITIONS  BY  THE   TRANSLATOE.  app.  xxT 

Table  I.— Historical  Pitches  ik  order  from  Lowest  to  BiQBXffr^continued. 


Cents 


Date 


Observer 


Place  and  other  particulars 


264 

267 
269 
270 


272 


f 


273 

275 
276 

278 

»» 
279 

280 

»» 
i82 


^:  S 


285 

286 
287 


431-3 

4317 
4322 

4323 


433 


433-2 


433-6 


433'9 
434 


434*3 


434"5 
4347 


435 

>» 
435-2 
435*4 


435-9 
436 


4361 

436-5 
4367 
436-8 


288  ,  4369 


1625 

1826 

1854a 

1846c 


1 820c 


Lewis  . 

Fischer 

Delezenne 

E. 


E. 


1828 


1847 


1834 
1829 


18340 
1818 


1869 


1826 


1859 

1834a 

1859 


1868 
1802 
1846P 


1878 
1878 

1834c 

X845 
1740- 
1780 
1869 


E. 


Byolin  &  E. 


Scheibler 

Cagniard 

Tour 


de    la 


Scheibler 
McLeod  &  E. 


E. 

E. 


Naeke 


Fr.  Com. 
Scheibler 
Eoenig  &  E 


Cross  &  Miller 
Sarti    . 
E. 


E. 

E. 

Scheibler 

Delezenne 

E. 

R. 


TJie  Compromise  Pi£cA— continued. 

as  the  pitch  of  the  Italian  army  brass  bands, 
giving  £b456,  the  nearest  whole  number  to 
equal  £b456'i3,  which  would  correspond  to  the 
*  arithmetical '  pitch  C5 1 2 
Lavenham  (16^  miles  W.N.W.  of  Ipswich),  from  a 

famous  old  tenor  bell  sounding  (2288*4 
Paris,  Grand  Opera,  fork  given  by  Spontini 
Lille,  organ  of  St.  Andr6  repaired 
England,  old  fork  which  belonged  to  the  father  of 
Messrs.  Bryceson,  organ-builders,  and  had  not 
been    tuned    since    1848,  when  it  had    been 
sharpened  slightly 
London,  fork  approved  of  by  Sir  Qeorge  Smart, 
conductor  of  the  Philharmonic  Concerts,  in  pos- 
session of  Mr.  Hipkins,  |prom  c"5i8  using  mean- 
tone  temperament ;  if  equal  temperament  were 
used  it  would  give  0^435*4  and  be  a  30  years' 
anticipation  of  French  pitch.    Used  in  this  way 
it  is  Broadwoods'  lowest  pitch.    Long  sold  in 
shops  as  *  London  Philharmonic ' 
London,  Sir  G.  Smart's  own  Philharmonic  fork. 
Sir  G.  Smart  considered  this  a'  fork  of  his  to 
agree  with  c''5i8  (see  last  entry).    This  shews 
that  he  used  meantone  temperament 
Shrewsbury,  St.  Mary's,  built  1729  by  John  Harris 
and  John  Byfield,  pitch  altered  in  1847  ^7  Gtkj 
&  Davison 
Vienna,  fork  I.,  Delezenne's  Vienna  minimum 
Paris,  opera,  verified  by  M.  Montal,  after  the  opera 
had  recovered  its  pitch,  the  opera  piano  remain- 
ing at  a'425'5,  which  see,  and  also  a'425*8 
Paris  Opera,  fork  by  Petitbout,  luthier  de  I'opdra 
Paris,  Chapelle  des  Tuileries,  from  a  copy  com- 
pared by  Cavaill^-Coll  of  fork  No.  493  in  the 
Conservatoire 
Baden,  fork  sent  officially  to  Society  of  Arts 
London,  from  a  model  of  pipe  representing  6'486*i, 
one  foot  long  and  one  inch  diameter,  on  Benatus 
Harris's  organ  at  All  Hallows,  Barking 
Dresden,  opera,  fork  of  Kapellmeister  Beissiger, 
successor  to  C.  M.  von  Weber.    Naeke  considers 
this  to  have  been  Dresden  pitch  from  1825  to  1830 
Carlsruhe,  opera,  the  fork  which  determined  the 

French  Diapason  Normal 
Paris,  Conservatoire,  fork  made  by  Gand,  luthier 

du  Conservatoire 
Paris,  the  Diapason  Normal  in  the  Conservatoire, 
used  extensively  in  Germany,  officially  adopted 
for  the  Belgian  army  in  1885.    The  various  im- 
perfect copies  used  are  not  cited 
U.  S.  America,  E.  S.  Ritchie's  standard  pitch 
St.  Petersburg,  five-foot  organ  pipes 
London    *  Philharmonic,'    from    Mr.    Hipkins's 
vocal  pitch,  c"5i8*5,  which  for  equal  tempera- 
ment gives  a'436,  but  on  meantone  temperament, 
for  which  it  was  first  used,  gave  a'433'5 ;  the  fork 
with  which  Mr.  E.  J.  Hopkins  compmd  the  pitch 
of  the  organs  at  Liibeck,  Hamburg,  and  Strass- 
burg,  see  his  The  Organ  ed.  1870,  art.  791,  p.  189 
London,  Messrs.  Bishop's  standard  for  church 

organs 
London,  fork  to  which  Messrs.  Bryceson  tuned  the 

organ  at  Her  Majesty's  Theatre 
Vienna,  opera,  fork  U. 
Florence,  fork  lent  by  M.  Marloye 
Dublin,  Green's  organ  in  the  B^ectory  of  Trinity 

College,  probably  sharpened 
Wiirtemberg,  fork  sent  officially  to  the  Society  of 
Arts 


Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


SECT.  H.     THE   HISTORY  OP  MUSICAL  PITCH  IN  EUROPE.  499 

Tabus  I.— Historical  Pitches  in  order  from  Lowest  to  Kiqkebt— continued. 


Oenta 


Obserrer 


Placo  and  other  partioalars 


6.  Modem  Orchestral  Pitchy  and  *  Chwrch  Pitch  Medium, 


288 
2S9 

437 
4371 

1859 
1666 

tl 

437-3 

1872 

II 

4374 

1854a 

II 

II 

1744 

291 

437-8 

1862 

295 

438-9 

1696 

297 

439*4 

— 

»l 
II 

II 
II 

18340 
1878 

298 

439-5 

1813 

II 

II 

1855 

299 

439-9 
440 

1845 
1829 

II 

II 
440-2 

1878 
1834 

♦  I 
II 

302 
303 

II 
440-3 

440-5 
4409 

1879 
1834c 
1878 
18340 

304 

44I-0 

1836- 
1839 

II 

II 

1836 

II 

II 

1859 

tl 

»» 

1879 

II 

441*10 

1834 

305 

441-2 

1878 

II 

441*3 

1842 

307 

441*7 

1690 

l» 

II 

1660 

" 

II 

1878 

Fr.  Com.      . 
E. 

Fischer 

Delezenne    . 

Streatfield  &  £. 

E. 

E. 

Delezenne    . 

Scheibler 
E. 

MoLeod  <&  E. 


Delezenne 
Lissajons 

E. 
Scheibler 


E. 

Scheibler 
E. 
Scheibler 

Delezenne 


Gagniard     de 

Tour 
Fr.  Com. 

E. 

Scheibler 


E. 
E. 

E. 

E. 
E. 


Tooloase,  Conservatoire 
^Worcester,  cathedral  organ  built  by  Thomas  and 

Benatas  Harris,  from  a  pipe  at  Mr.  T.  Hill's 
Berlin,  from  a  fork  furnished  by  Pichler,  who 

tnned  the  piano  of  the  opera 
Paris,  opera,  from  four  forks  purchased  before 

1854,  and  found  to  be  in  unison 
^Maidstone,  Old  Parish  Churdi,  built  by  Jordans, 

altered,  but  not  in  pitch,  in  1878  in  meantone 

temperament 
Dresden,  fork  given  by  the  direction  of  the  Court 

Theatre  to  its  librarian,  Herr  Moritz  Furstenau, 

after  the  conference  on  pitch  held  there,  by  whom 

it  was  lent  me  to  measure,  meant  for  a'440 
'Boston,  England,  organ  built  by  Christian  Smith, 

from  a  pipe  preserved  by  Mr.  T.  Hill 
Lille,  old  fork  formerly  belonging  to  the  Marquis 

d'Aligre 
Vienna,  opera,  fork  in. 
Dresden,  opera  pitch  at  date,  from  a  fork  specially 

prepared  for  me  by  the   Court  organ-builder, 

Jehmlich,  and  sent  by  Herr  Moritz  Fiirstenau, 

librarian  of  the  theatre 
Paris,  Conservatoire,  from  copy  of  a  fork  preserved 

there,  verified  by  Cavaill6-Coll 
England,  Barking,  Essex,  Parish  Church  organ 

(probably  originally  a'474'i),  built  by  By  field  & 

Green,  1770,  after  alterations  by  Messrs.  Walker 
Turin,  fork  lent  by  Marloye 
Paris,  opera  orchestra,  verified  by  Monneron  for 

de  la  Fage 
London,  Messrs.  Gray  A  Davison's  standard  pipe 
Stuttgart  pitch,  =440  at  69^  F.,  Lissajous  mea- 
sured it  as  440*3  to  French  Diapason  Normal^ 

reckoned  as  435,  which  then  when  corrected  to 

435*4  gives  4407 
London,  Messrs.  Walker  &  Sons*  standard  pipe 
Vienna  Opera,  fork  IV. 
London,  Messrs.  Bevington's  standard  pipe 
Paris  Conservatoire,  not    trusted  so   much    by 

Scheibler  as  435'2 
Paris  Opera,  fork  of  M.  Leibner,  who  kept  the 

pianos    to    pitch    of    orchestra,    verified     by 

Meyerbeer 
Paris,  Op^ra  Comique 

Dresden,  fork  sent  to  Fr.  Com.  by  the  Kapell- 
meister Beissiger 

London,  church  organ  pitch  of  Messrs.  Lewis  & 
Co. 

Vienna  Opera,  fork  V.,  given  by  Prof.  Blahetka  as 
trustworthy ;  in  1879  this  fork  was  found  and 
lent  to  me,  and  then  from  rust  and  ill>treatment 
measured  only  439*9,  the  greatest  loss  of  pitch 
I  have  found  in  any  fork 

London,  Covent  Garden  Opera,  f6rk  for  Messrs. 
Bryceson  to  tune  the  organ  to 

London,  the  equal  a'  corresponding  to  the  late 
Dr.  John  Hullah's  standard  fork,  c"524*8,  pur- 
porting to  be  c"5i2 ;  J.  H.  Griesbach  measured 
it  as  521*6 

Hampton    Court    Palace,   Bernhardt    Schmidt's 
organ  from  an  original  pipe,  12  inches  long  and  ; 
I  '2  inch  in  diameter,  giving  1/  b472  *6 

Whitehall,  Chapel  Boyal,  organ  by  Bernhardt 
Schmidt,  according  to  indications  by  Mr.  T.  Hill . 

London,  standard  pipe  of  Messrs.  Hill  and  Sons, 
from  c"525-3 


H 


ir 


Digitized  by 


v55bgie 


500  ADDITIONS  BY   THE  TRANSLATOR.  app.  m. 

Table  I. — Historical  Pitchkb  in  ordbb  pbom  Lowest  to  BiaHEvr— continued. 


Date 


Observer 


Place  and  other  partioalars 


6.  Modem  Orchestral  Pitchy  and  *  Church  Pitch  Medium — oontintied. 


307 
310 

441-8 
442-5 

1834c 
1859 

*3ii 

n 
4427 

1878 

312 

443-0 

1859 

443-1 

1815c 
1869 

313 

443-2 

1878 

»* 

n 

n 
314 
315 

443-3 

443-4 
443-5 
443*9 

1836 

III? 

»» 

444 

i860 

316 

444-2 

1880 

317 

444-3 

1840 

»» 

»i 

1880 

318 

444-5 

1858 

»» 

444-6 

1877 

»» 

444-7 

1879 

319 

444-8 

1859 

t« 

ti 

»l 

»» 

»t 
320 

»i 
321 

444-9 

445-0 

It 
445-1 

1862 
1834c 

»i 

445-2 

1878 

322 
it 

445-4 
445-5 

1845 
1879 

It 

445-6 

" 

*• 

445-8 

1867 

1856 

Scheibler 
Fr.  Com. 

t» 
E. 


Fr.  Com. 

♦» 
E. 

E. 

E. 

W61fel  . 

Fr.  Com. 

E. 

Fr.  Com. 

Cross  &  Miller 


Cavaill6-Coll 
E. 


Lissajous 

E. 

E. 

Fr.  Com. 


LissajoQS 
Hipkins 
Naeke  . 
Sohmahl 
Scheibler 

E. 


Delezemie     . 
Hipkins  &  E. 

E. 


E. 

Lissajous 


Berlin  opera 

Toulouse  opera 

Brussels,  opera  under  direction  of  Bender 

*Vienna,  small  Franciscan  organ  kept  at  modem 

pitch,  from  a  fork  tuned  for  me  by  the  organ- 
builder  UUmann 
Bordeaux  opera 
Stuttgart  opera 
*Durham  organ,  as  altered  by  shifting  from  a'474*  i ; 

a'4447,  the  present  pitch  of  new  organ,  is  by 

Willis 
Bologna,  Italy,  Liceo  Musicale,  from  fork   sent 

officially  to  Soc.  of  Arts 
^Vienna,  St.  Stefan  cathedral  organ,  from  a  fork 

tuned  for  me  by  organ-builder  Ullmann. 
Paris,  Wdlfel's  pianos 
Gotha,  opera 

London,  from  Messrs.  Bryceson's  standard  pipe 
Brunswick,  opera 
U.S.  America,  Boston,  organ  of  Church   of  the 

Immaculate  Conception 
Intended  but  unexecuted  standard  of  Society  of 

Arts  to  c"528 
U.S.  America,  from  c"528,  the  •  low  organ  pitch  * 

of  Hutchings,  Plaisted  A  Co. 
^France,  St    Denis    Cathedral,    organ  built    by 

Cavaill^-CoU 
^London,  Temple  Church  organ  after  rebuilding 

by  Messrs.  Forster  and  Andrews,  who  retained 

the  pitch  which  they  found,  which  was  Bobom's, 

originally  built   by  Bernhardt  Schmidt,   with 

both  Eb  and  Dff ,  and  both  ^b  and  G8  keys, 

and  perhaps  then  having  a'4417 
Madrid,  Theatre  Royal,  fork  sent  to  de  la  Page  by 

the    Maitre  de   Chapelle.    French    pitch   was 

adopted  on  18  March  1879 
♦London,  St.  PauPs,  after  rebuilding  by  Willis, 

from    a    fork    belonging   to    Mr.    Hipkins   at 

57°-5 

♦Durham  Cathedral  organ,  rebuilt  by  Willis ;  for 
its  original  state,  see  a'474'i 

Turin  opera 

Weimar  opera 

Wiirtemberg  concerts 

Naples,  San  Carlo  opera,  Gxiillaume*s  fork 

London,  Her  Majesty's  opera,  fork  of  the  theatre 

Vienna,  piano  of  Kapellmeister  Proch 

Hamburg  *  old  pitch,'  date  unknown 

Vienna  opera,  fork  VI.,  *a  monstrous  growth* 
(Auswitchs)  in  Scheibler's  opinion 

♦London  {from  c"529*4),  Mr.  H.  Willis's  church 
pitch,  to  which  he  tuned  the.  organs  of  the 
cathedrals  of  St.  Paul's  (London),  Durham, 
Salisbury,  Glasgow  (established),  St.  Mary 
(Edinburgh) 

Vienna  Conservatorium,  fork  lent  by  Marloye 

London,  Her  Majesty's  opera  during  pOTform- 
anoe 

London,  Covent  Garden  opera,  fork  in  possession 
of  Mr.  Pitman,  organist,  and  Sig.  Vianed,  con- 
ductor. Mr.  Pitman  said  the  pitch  was  thus  in 
1878  because  oboe,  bassoon,  and  flute  would  not 
play  lower 

London,  Exeter  Hall,  both  organs  as  originally 
built,  from  a  pipe   at    the   makers',   Messrs.  > 
Walker ;  since  sharpened  to  a'447'3 

Paris  opera,  from  the  fork  of  M.  Bodin,  professor 
of  the  piano  and  music 


Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


$ECT.  H.     THE   HISTORY  OF  MUSICAL  PITCH  IN   EUROPE.  501 

Table  I.— Historical  Pitches  in  order  from  Lowest  to  Hiquzst— continued. 


Oento 


Date 


Obseryer 


Place  and  other  particulars 


6.  Modem  Orchestral  Pitch,  and  *  Church  Pitch  Medium— continued. 


445*9 


446 
4462 

446*6 
446-8 


1849- 
1854 


1859 
1856 
1859 
1845 
1851 
1878 


447-0    1859 
447*3    1879 


447*4 
447*5 
4477 


448 


448-1 


1856 
1878 
1877 


1854 
1839 
1840 
1859 


448-2;  1869 


448-4 


448-5 

448-8 
449 

449*2 
449-4 

449*7 

449*8 
449*9 
450*3 
450*5 


450-6 

450-9 
451*5 
451*7 


451*9 
452 


1857 
i860 

1880 

1859 
1855 

1877 
i860 

1879 

1859 
1877 
1856 
1848 
&1854 

1877 


1880 
1858 
1874 


1867 
1880 

1878 

1885 


£.  .  .  .  ;  London,  from  Broadwoods*  original  mediom  pitch 
of  c"530-6,  fork  of  the  tuner  Finlayson ;  since 
1854  Messrs.  Broadwoods  use  a'446-2  as  their  me- 
dium pitch.    This  pitch  was  chosen  empirically 

Pr.  Com.       .        .     Pesth,  opera 

Lissajous      .        .     Paris,  opera  and  Ck)n8ervatoire 

Fr.  Com.       .        .     Holland,  the  Hague  at  the  Conservatoire 

Delezenne     .        .     Milan,  fork  lent  by  Marloye 

„  Lille,  festival  organ,  fork  of  the  tuner  Mazingue 

E.  .  .  .  Vienna  opera,  from  a  fork  sent  me  by  the  organ- 
builder,  Ullmann,  who  had  charge  of  the  organ 
there 

Fr.  Com.       .        .     Marseilles  Conservatoire 

E.         .  .     London,  Exeter  Hall  organ,  from  a  pipe  of  the 

makers,  Messrs.  Walker,  see  445*8 

Lissajous      .  Paris,  Italian  opera,  Bodin's  fork 

Hipkins        .  London,  Covent  Garden  opera  harmonium 

E.  .  .  .  Gloucester  Festival  organ,  built  by  Messrs.  Walker ; 
from  the  fork  to  which  it  was  tuned  at  64°  F., 
the  temperature  of  the  pipe  being  reduced  to  59° 

Lissajous      .  Paris,  Grand  Opera— also  at  Lyons  and  Lidge 

Schmahl       .        .     Hamburg,  opera,  under  Erebs 

Fr.  Com.       .        .     Munich,  opera 

E.         .        .  Leipzig,  Qewandhaus  Concerts,  from  fork  sent  offi- 

cially to  the  Society  of  Arts 

Lissajous      .        .     Berlin,  opera,  fork  of  the  conductor  Taubert 

E.  .  .  .  London,  from  Gramer*s  c"533*3,  purporting  to  be 
the  Society  of  Arts'  pitch,  intended  for  c''528 

Cross  &  Miller  Boston,  Nichol's  fork  of  Germania  Orchestra,  as 

corrected  to  59®  F. 

Fr.  Com.       .        .     Leipzig  Conservatoire 

Lissajous      .  Paris  opera,  experiments  by  Lissajous  and  Fer- 

rand,  the  first  violin 

Hipkins  .     Covent  Garden  Opera,  pitch  of  the  harmonium 

£.  .  .  .  London,  from  Griesbach's  0^534*5,  tuned  for  the 
Society  of  Arts  as  c''528  ;  he  tuned  a'  as  4457 

Hipkins  .  .  London,  Covent  Garden  opera,  taken  from  organ 
a*  during  performance 

Fr.  Com.       .        .     Prague,  opera  • 

E.         .        .        .     London,  firom  copy  of  CoUard's  standard  fork 

Lissajous      .        .     Milan,  opera 

Delezenne  .  .  Lille,  from  forks  tuned  by  the  oboist  Colin,  during 
the  performances  of  Robert  le  Diahle,  27  April 
1854,  between  the  acts,  and  carefully  venfied 

E.  •  .  .  Glasgow  Public  Halls  organ,  from  fork  settled  by 
the  organist  W.  T.  Best  and  the  late  H.  Smart, 
lent  me  by  the  builder  Lewis 

Cross  &  Miller  U.S.  America,  Boston  Music  Hall,  reduced  from 

pipe  0271-2  at  70**  F. 

Fr.  Com.  .  .  Bussian  opera,  from  a  cf'  fork,  probably  miscalcu- 
lated, as  the  a'  from  Broadwoods*  c"  forks  were 

E.  .  •  .  Belgian  army  pitch,  reduced  from  Eoenig*s  451 
vib.  by  his  old  standard,  and  also  measured 
from  copy  sent  by  Mahillon.  On  19  March  1885 
the  Belgian  Ck)vernment  adopted  French  pitch, 

^435 

Lissajous  Milan,  Scala  Theatre 

Cross  &  Miller  U.S.  America,  New  York,  from  Chickering's  0268*5 

standard  fork 

E.  .  .  .  British  Army  regulation,  from  fork  lent  by  Dr. 
W.  H.  Stone 

E.         .        .  The  International  Inventions  and  Music  Exhibi- 

tion of  1885  adopted  this  as  the  pitch  of  all 
instruments  for  the  exhibition,  being  the  near- 
est whole  number  to  the  next  preceding  and 
next  following.   The  fork  was  verified  by  myself 

Digitized  by  V^OOQ IC 


502  ADDITIONS  BY  THE  TKANSLATOR-  app.  tt 

Tablx  I. — HisTomcAi.  Pitches  in  obdxu  fbom  Lowest  to  Hiohbst — continued. 


Cents 


Date 


Obeeirer 


Place  and  other  parttcnlars 


6.  Modem  Orchestral  Pitcht  and  *  Church  Pitch  Medium — continaed. 


349     452-5 


f 


350 


354 


355 


357 


358 
359 


362 


452*9 

453 

4539 

454 

4541 


1852- 
1874 


1880 
1878 
1645 
1878 

1862 
1877 


454*2    1 71 50 


4547 


4551 
455*2 

455*3 
455'5 

455*9 
456-1 


366 

369 
380 


457*2 

458-0 
460-8 


E. 


Chambers  &  £. 
E. 

Sohmahl 
E. 


Naeke 


E. 


1874 


1879 
1878 


1877 

1749 

1879 
1859 

1877 
1880 

1859a 


1879 
1880 


Hipkins  &  £. 

Sohmahl  A  E, 

E. 

Pr.  CJom. 

E.  •        • 

Cross  &  Miller 


B, 


Cross  &  Miller 


>i  ft 


London,  mean  of  the  pitch  of  the  Philharmonic 
Band  onder  the  direction  of  Sir  Michael  Costa 
1846-54,  tuned  during  that  period  by  Mr.  J. 
Black  of  Broadwoods*,  approved  by  Sir  Bfichael 
Costa,  and  recorded  by  Mr.  Hipkins,  who  lent 
me  the  fork.  Used  as  Broadwoods'  highest  till 
1874, 1^0.  3  of  French  Conunission 
NewcasUe-on-Tyne,  Schulze's  Tynedock  organ, 
from  a  fork  tuned  by  Bir.  Ch.  Chambers,  Mus.  B. 
Eneller  Hall  Training  School  for  Military  Music, 

from  a  fork  lent  by  Dr.  W.  H.  Stone 
*Holstein,  Gliickstadt  organ  bcult  1645,  improved 

by  Schnitger  1665,  measured  1879 
London,  Wi]lis*B  concert  organ  pitch,  to  which  he 
tuned  the  large  organs  in  the  Albert  Hall  and 
Alexandra  Palace,  from  pipe  c''543*2  at  65^  F., 
and  54r2  at  61*5**  F. 
Vienna,  piano  of  EapeUmeister  Esser,  while  the 
orchestra  was  at  a'466,  the    regular  fork  at 
a'456*i,  and  the  piano  of  the  other  Kapellmeister 
Prooh  at  a'445 
Crystal  Palace,  from  a  fork  cf'SAO  lent  by  Mr. 
Hipkins,  to  which  the  piano  for  concerts  was 
tuned 
London,  very  old  fork  found  at  Brixton  1878  of 
the  same  make  as  Bev.  G.  T.  Driffield's  tenor  a, 
see  a'4i9*9 
London,  from  0^540*8,  a  fork  representing  the 
highest  pitch  of  the  London  Philharmonic  ob- 
served by  Mr.  Hipkins  since  1874 ;  at  the  sng- 
gestion  of  Mr.  Charles  Hall6,  used  as  Broad- 
wood's  highest  pitch 
London,  Messrs.  Steinway*s  London  pitch 
London,  Messrs.  Bryoeson's  band  pitch,  to  which 
they  tuned  their  organ  in  St.  Michael's,  Corn- 
hill,  London 
London,  Wagner  Festival  at  Albert  Hall,  tempe- 
rature probably  61*5**  F.,  see  above  ^'453*9 
Hamburg,  old  positiv  or  chamber  organ,  buiU  by 

Lehnert,  in  possession  of  Herr  Schmahl 
London,  Erard's  concert  pitch,  from  their  fork 
Belgium,  band  of  Guides ;  probably  no  such  fork 
existed.    M.  Bender  used  to  give  the  pitch  on  a 
small  darinet,  from  which  M.  Mahillon  has  a 
fork  of  at  least  a'456 
London,  fork  used  by  one  of  Chappell's  toners, 

lent  me  by  Dr.  Stone 
U.S.  America,  Cincinnati,  pitch  used  in  Thomas's 
orchestra.     [This  is  said  by  de  la  Fage  to  have 
been  the  pitch  sent  by  Bettini  in  1857  for  the 
London  Italian  opera— evidently  an  error} 
Vienna,  fork    tuned  for  me  by  the   pianoforte 
makers  Streicher  in  Vienna   from  a  fork  in 
their  possession,  giving  the  celebrated  'sharp 
Vienna  pitch '  before  the  introduction  of  the 
French  Diapaeon  Normal,   Naeke  says  he  heard 
a'466  in  the  actual  playing  of  the  orchestra 
U.S.  America,  New  Tork,  from  a  fork  obtained  for 
me  bv  Messrs.  Steinway  as  representing  their 
American  pitch 
U.S.  America,  New  Tork,  from  a  fork  furnished 

by  B.  Spice  as  Steinway's  pitch 
U.S.  America,  highest  New  York  pitch,  from  a 
fork  furnished  by  B.  Spice ;  these  two  last  are 
sharper  than  the  next,  but  they  are  put  first 
because  they  belong  to  modem  orchestral  or 
pianoforte  pitch. 


Digitized  by 


Google 


ffiCT.  H.     THE  HISTOBY  OF  MUSICAL  PITCH  IN  EUEOPE.  503 

Tablb  I. — Historical  Pitches  in  order  prom  Lowest  to  Highest — contiwued. 


Gents 


Observer 


Place  and  other  particulars 


7.  Church  Pitch,  High, 


368 


429 


454 


465 


484 


457-6 


474*1 


I 640c  E, 


1668 


»» 

1683 

»» 

1708 

»l 

1748 

480*8 

1879 

484-1 

1878 

4892 

1688 

E. 


Armes  &  E. 


E. 


Degcnhardt  &  E. 


Jimmerthal 


Sohmahl  &  E. 


Vienna,  Great  Franciscan  organ,  stated  by  organ- 
builder  Ullmann  to  be  240  years  old  in  1878, 
and  to  possess  its  original  pitch ;  only  used  for 
leading  the  ecclesiastical  chants 

England,  in  the  Pars  Organica  of  Tomkins's 
Musica  Deo  Sacra  as  quoted  in  Sir  F.  A.  Gore 
Ouseley's  Collectum  of  the  Compositions  of 
Orlando  Oibhons,  1873,  makes  the  /  pipe 
2^  feet  long 

Durnam,  Bernhardt  Schmidt's  original  organ  at 
Durham,  which  had  both  ab  and  gZ  •  The 
pipe  I  measured  in  Feb.  1879  ^  ^'443*1  ^^  ^^^ 
shifted,  and  was  orginally  g%  ,  which  gives  the 
above  pitch.  This  results  from  an  examina- 
tion of  the  original  pipes  by  Dr.  Armes,  the 
organist 

Chapel  Boyal,  St.  James's,  Bernhardt  Schmidt's 
organ,  now  in  Mercers'  Hall,  which  I  found  on 
examination  had  had  the  pipes  shifted  a  great 
Semitone.  Handel  played  on  this  organ,  and 
hence  his  note  ordering  the  voice  parts  of  an 
anthem  written  for  the  Chapel  Boyal  to  be 
transposed  one  Tone,  and  the  organ  part  ttvo 
Tones,  referred  to  this  organ 

The  Jordans'  organ,  Botolph  Lane,  from  indica- 
tions by  Mr.  T.  Hill 

Hamburg,  St.  Catherine  Eirche,  built  by  Hans 
Stellwagen  in  1543,  and  frequently  repaired. 
Herr  Degenhardt,  the  organist,  declares  that 
even  at  the  last  repairing,  1867-9,  the  pitch  was 
not  altered.  The  original  pitch,  however,  is 
doubtful,  and  Herr  Sohmahl  thinks  it  was 
altered  formerly 

Lubeok  Cathedral,  small  organ,  which  according 
to  the  organist  Jimmerthal  has  its  g'  in  unison 
with  the  pipe  on  Schulze's  new  great  organ 
there,  whidi  gives  French  a'  in  summer  at  68®  F. ; 
whence  the  above  was  calculated  at  59°  F. 

Hamburg,  St.  Jacob!  Eirche,  built  by  Schnitger 
of  Harburg  originally  in  equal  temperament, 
played  on  and  approved  by  J.  Sebastian  Bach ; 
pitch  determined  from  an  old  pipe  preserved  in 
the  organ  case.  Herr  Schmahl  the  organist  is 
accustomed  to  transpose  all  music  at  sight  one 
Tone  lower,  which  brings  it  to  French  pitch 


8.  Church  Pitch,  Highest. 


502 

494*5 

1879 

506 

495*5 

1700 

534 

5037 

1636 

535 

504-2 

1511 

541 

505-8 

1 361 

Schmahl  &  E. 

Schmahl 
E. 

E. 


Hamburg,  St.  Jaoobi  Eirche,  present  pitch,  used 
since  1866  in  order  to  agree  with  Sdieibler's 
forks,  taking  his  a'440  for  g' 

Holstein,  B6ndsburg,a  large  organ  recently  broken 
up 

Paris,  Mersenne's  ton  de  chapelle  with  G 11 2*6  on 
the  Frendi  four-foot  pipe,  this  being  the  lowest 
note  of  his  own  voice 

Heidelberg,  from  a  model  after  Arnold  Schlick, 
who  recommends  that  his  6^-foot  Bhenish  pipe, 
having  301*6  vib.,  should  give  i^  or  c.  If  it  gives 
F  we  have  a'377,  if  it  gives  c  we  have  the  pre- 
sent pitch 

Halberstadt  organ,  built  1361,  repaired  1495,  de- 

.  scribed  by  PrsBtorius,  who  gives  the  dimensions 
of  the  largest  pipe  IB,,,,  whence  constructing  a 
model  I  arrived  at  the  above  pitch,  confirmed 
by  the  four  preceding  pitches 


1[ 


Digitized  by 


^.joogle 


5^4 


Cents 


ADDITIONS  BY  THE  TRANSLATOR.  app.  xx, 

Table  I. — Historical  Pitches  in  order  from  Highest  to  Lowest — continued. 

Date 


Observer 


Place  and  other  particulars 


9.  Cha/mber  Pitchy  Highest^  and  Chu/rck  Pitchy  Extreme, 


726 


740 


563-1     1636     E. 


567-3 


1619 


Paris,  Mersenne's  chamber  pitch  calculated  from 
F  being  the  pipe  of  4  French  feet  giving  1 12*6 
vib.  See  Harmonie  UnwerseUe,  liv.  3,  p.  143, 
but  from  faulty  measurement  Mersenne  makes 
this  pipe  to  have  only  96  vib.  But  even  with 
that  assumption  the  pitch  would  be  a'48cri,  as 
at  Hamburg,  St.  Catherine  Kirche ;  but  compare 
the  next  entry 

North  German  church  pitch,  called  by  Pnetorius 
chamber  pitch,  taken  as  a  meantone  Fourth 
(503  cents)  above  Prstorius's  *  suitable  pitch ' 
a'424'2,  which  see 


Table  II. — Classified  Index  to  Table  I. 

The  countries  are  arranged  in  alphabetical  order:  L  Austro-Hungary;  £1.  Belgium; 
IIL  England,  including  Scotland  and  Lreland ;  IV.  France;  V.  Germany;  YI.  Holland;  VH. 
Italy ;  YIH.  Bussia ;  IX.  Spain ;  X.  United  States  of  America,  which  for  musical  purposes 
are  included  in  Europe. 

Under  each  country  the  pitches  are  classified  as:  i.  Standards;  2,  Old  Forks;  3.  Church 
Organs;  4.  Concert  Organs;  5.  Operas;  6.  Concerts,  including  Conservatoriums ;  7.  Piano- 
fortes ;  8.  Military  Music ;  9.  Other  instruments. 

The  cents  and  pitch  are  as  in  the  former  table,  to  which,  therefore,  immediate  reference  can 
be  made. 

Within  each  division  the  pitches  are  arranged  first  geographidally  and  then  ohronologieaUy, 
but  for  England  the  organs  by  the  same  makers  are  generally  put  together. 

The  mark  „  means  that  the  number  or  date  above  is  to  be  repeated,  and  —  that  the  date 
or  place  is  unknown. 
I        The  pitches  are  cited  with  the  greatest  brevity  which  will  allow  of  identification. 


Date 


Place 


Pitch 


Cento 


164OC 

1780 

1878 


1834* 


1878 
1859 


1845 


1780 
1862 


1879 


Vienna 


Pesth 
Prague 


Vienna 


Brussels 


I.  AUSTBO-HUNOABY. 

3.  Chu/rch  Organs, 


Large  Franciscan  organ 
Organ-builder  Schulz 
St.  Stefan 


368 
225 

313 
Small  Franciscan  organ 311 

5.  Opera, 
Scheibler,  fork     I. |  276 


n. 
„   in.       .      ,      . 

„     IV.  .        .        . 

„       V.  (Blahetka) 
„  „     VI.  (*  monstrosity ') 

Vienna  Old  Sharp  Pitch  . 
Ullmann ...... 

Fr.  Com 


6.  Concerts, 
Marloye  (Conservatoire)  . 


7.  Pianofortes. 


Stein,  for  Mozart 
Esser,  per  Naeke 
Proch,       „ 


286 
298 
301 
304 
321 
362 
327 
323 
338 


3^1 


226 

354 
320 


457-6 
421*3 
443*2 
442*7 


433*9 
436-5 
439*4 
440*3 
441-1 

445*1 
4561 
446-8 
446-0 
449-8 


445*4 


421-6 
454-0 
445-0 


II.  Belgium. 

I.  Standards,  I         | 

Mahillon's  Army  Standard      •       .       .        .        .1  345  |  451*7 

Digitized  by  V^OOQIC 


SECT.  H.     THE  HISTOBY  OF  MUSICAL  PITCH  IN  EUROPE. 
Tablb  n. — Classifibd  Indbx  to  Tabue  l.—ooniinutd. 


5«>S 


Place 


Cents 


1859 


1842 
i860 


1877a 
I877P 


17150 

18000 
18460 


1625 
1668ft 

1660 

1683 
181SP 

1879a 

1690 

♦» 

1708 

1759 
1683 
1879 

1666 

1670 
1696 
1700 
1878a 

1778 
1780 
1788 
1790 


1696 

1749 

1730 
1820a 


BrnsBels 

BniBsels 
London 


II.  Bbloium-  oon^nued. 
5.  Opera. 
Bender's  pitoh 

6.  Concerts, 
Conservatoire  .... 


8.  Military  Instruments. 
Band  of  GoldeB  (Fr.  Ck>m.) 

III.  England,  Scotland,  and  Ireland. 

I.  Standards. 

Hullah'Bc"5i2,  really  524-8    . 
Society  of  Arts  intended  c"528 
Oriesbach's  attempt  at  c"528  a  534*5 
Griesbaoh's  a'  to  his  c"    . 

Cramer's  a'  and  c" 

Tonic  Solf  a  College  .        .        .        . 


Plymouth 
London 


Lavenham 
London 


Durham 


Hampton  Court 

t» 

London 

Cambridge 

Temple 


Worcester 

Newcastle 
London 

»i 
Norwich 

Londoii 

Winchester 

Windsor 

Kew 
Dublin 

Boston,  Line. 

London 

Westminster 


2.  Old  Forks. 

Faraday's        .... 
Bev.  G.  T.  DriflSeld's  a    . 
Fork  found  buried  at  Brixton,  a 
Handel's  own  fork  . 
Broadwoods'  c"        .        .        . 
Dr.  Stainer's  a'        .        .        . 
Bryceson's  c"  ,  "      . 


Church  Organs  and  BellSt  and  Organ-builders^ 
Church  Standards. 


Church  Bell  (2'288-4 

Tomkins's  Bule       .... 

Bernhardt  Schmidt : 

Whitehall,  original   . 
„  altered    . 

Original 

(Altered) 

(New,  by  Willis) 

Chapel 

Old  pipe  of  original  . 

St.  James's  Chapel  Boyal,  original 

Trinity  College,  after  shifting  . 

Original 

Altered 

T.  A  R.  Harris  : 

Cathedral         .... 
Renatus  Harris : 

St.  Nicholas      .... 

St.  Andrew  Undershaft    . 

St.  John's,  derkenwell    . 

(?)  Cathedral     .... 
Green: 

St.  Katharine's,  Regent's  Park 

Restoration  of  College  organ    . 

St.  George's  Chapel . 

Parish  Church  .... 

Trinity  CoUege  (altered  ?) 
Christian  Smith  : 

Parish  Church  (restored  ?) 
Olyn  &  Parker : 

All  Hallows  the  Great  and  Less 
Schreider  d  Jordans : 

Original 

(Altered) 


310 
331 
359 


317 
280 


295 
^Z7 


230 


442-5 
448*0 

455-5 


305 

441-3 

310 

444-0 

337 

4495 

322 

4457 

333 

448*4 

250 

427-5 

230 

422-5 

237 

424-3 

219 

419-9 

355 

454-2 

230 

4225 

231 

422-7 

23X 

4246 

270 

432-3 

265 

431-3 

429 

474-1 

3^ 

44X-7 

429 

474-1 

312 

443-1 

318 

444-7 

308 

442*0 

307 

441-7 

429 

474-1 

114 

395-2 

444-3 

437-1 


25s 

428*7 

251 

427*7 

215 

419-0 

249 

4272 

233 

423*2 

220 

420*1 

251 

427*8 

230 

422-6 

287 

436*8 

438-9 
424-3 


422*5 


Digitized  by  V^OOQIC 


5o6 


ADDITIONS  BY  THE  TRANSLATOR. 
Table  £1.— Clabsifibd  Index  to  Table  L— continued. 


AFP.  XX. 


Date 


Pitch 


Cents 


I740P 
1764 

London 
Halifax 

1855 

Barking 
It 

1826 
1847 

Shrewsbury 
»i 
II 

1740 

Gt.  Yarmouth 

I74« 

Maidstone 
London 

1838 

Bath 

1843 

Wimbledon 

1878 

London 

— 

Newcastle 

1879a 
»» 
It 
It 

Salisbury 

Glasgow 

Edinburgh 

London 

1878 


m. 


England,  Scotland,  and  Isslajhd— continued. 
3.  Church  Organs,  rfc—continued. 
Schnetsler : 

German  Chapel  Royal 

Parish  Church 

By  field  d  Green  : 

Original  probably     .        .        .        .  *     . 

(Restored  by  Walker) 

/.  Byfield  d  J,  Harris  : 

Original 

(Altered  by  Blythe) 

(Altered  by  Gray  &  Davison)    .... 
Byfield,  Jordan,  d  Bridge : 

St.  Gorge's  Chapel 

Jordans: 

Old  Parish  Church 

St.  George's,  Botolph  Lane       .... 

Parish  Church  (altered?)         .        .        .        . 
Smith  of  Bristol : 

Abbey  Church 

Walker : 

Parish  Church 

Bryceson : 

St.  Michael's,  Comhill 

Schulze  : 

Tynedock 

H.  Willis  : 

Cathedral 

Established  Church  Cathedral 

Episcopalian  Cathedral 

St.  Paul's,  present  state  (like  the  other  three 

at  59°  F.,  but)  at  S7°'5  P 

Organ-builders*  Standard  Pipe, 

Bishop,  c"5 18-5 

Gray  <&  Davison,  c"523*2 

Walker,  c"523-6 

Bevington,  c"5237 

Lewis,  c"524*4 

Hill.c"S25-3 

Bryceson,  c"527*3 

H.  Willis  (church),  c"S29-4 

Experimental  English  i-foot  Pipes. 
Diam.  1*2  inch ;  wind  2|  inch. ;  vib.  477-0 
taken  as  c"    gives] 

"      *l/b      "    \  ^  meantone  temperament  ■ 

»t      n  »'         »•    ) 
Same  diam. ;  wind  3^  inch. ;  vib.  478*7 
taken  as  c"    gives 

in  meantone  temperament 


1/ 

6'b 

a' 


Bernhardt  Schmidt's,  same  dimensions;  wind  2^ 
inch. ;   vib.  472*9 
taken  as  &'   gives 


It       n   ^ 

II      tt  ^b 
,.     ,1  a' 
Diam.  '95  inch ;  wind  3^  inch, 
taken  as  c"   gives 
ti       It  o  ,, 

tt     It  ^b     ti 
It     It  ^        It 
Diam.  *75  inch. ;  wind  3^  inch. ;  vib.  498*6 
taken  as  c''   gives) 


in  meantone  temperament 

vib.  488*7 
in  meantone  temperament 


b' 

b'b 

a' 


in  meantone  temperament 


Digitized  by 


Goo^ 


242 


429 
298 


275 
244 

289 

424 
262 

230 

246 

357 

350 

320 
II 
ti 

318 

284 
300 
301 
302 
304 
307 
313 
320 


133 
247 
323 
440 

136 
253 
329 
446 


"5 
231 
308 

425 

171 
289 

482 

206 

323 
400 
516 


lECT.  H.     THE  fflSTORY  OP  MUSICAL  PITCH  IN  EUROPE. 
Table  II. — Classitisd  Indxx  to  Tabus  L—contiwued. 


507 


Date 


Place 


Pitch 


Cents 


1805 

1867 
1879 
1877 


1877a 
1877 


1857 


1877 


1878 


London 

GloQoester 

Glasgow 

London 

»» 

Sydenham 
London 


1877 

>» 

1878 

l> 

St 

If 

1879 

»» 

1880 

If 
ft 

1878 

ft 

1879 

If 

1880 

ft 

'^11- 

ft 
If 

1826 

ft 

1846- 

f» 

1854 

1874 

ft 

1877 

Sydenham 

»» 

London 

1826 

It 

1849- 

ft 

1854 

I854P 

>• 

i860 

ft 

1852- 

tf 

1874 

I874P 

ft 

1846a 

f> 

I846P 

If 

1877 

ft 

1879 

If 

ni.  Enolamd,  Scotland,  and  Ibeland — continued, 

4.  Concert  Organs. 
ElUott: 

Ancient  Concerts  from  d"$6&'2 
Walker : 

Exeter  Hall,  original 

„        „      sharpened 

Festival  organ 

Lewis: 

Public  Halls 

H.  WilUs : 

Concert  Standard  at  Albert  Hall  and  Alex- 
andra Palace 

Albert  Hall,  observed  at  61-5^  F.      . 
Gray  A  Davison : 

Crystal  Palace 

Bryceson: 

Band  pitch 

5.  Opera, 

Opera,  Bettini's  fork  (correct?)  .  .  .  . 
Covent  Qa/rden, : 

Harmoniom 

Organ  (Bryceson's  fork) 

Harmoniom 

Organ  (heard) 

Band  (performing) 

Theatre  fork  (season  1880)  .... 
Her  Majesty^s : 

Organ 

Band  (performing) 

Theatre  fork 


6.  Concerts. 
Philharmonic : 

Copy  of  original  fork 

Another  copy 

Approved  by  Sir  G.  Smart        .... 

Mean  pitch  while  the  concerts  were  under  the 

direction  of  Sir  M.  Costa       .... 

Highest 

Crystal  Palace  band 

Wagner  Festival  at  Albert  Hall 

7.  Pianofortes. 

Broadwoods'  lowest,  London  No.  i  of  Fr.  Com. 
medium,  London,  No.  2  of  Fr.  Com. 

copy  now  used 

copy  made  for  Society  of  Arts  .... 

highest,  London  No.  3  of  Fr.  Com.  (which 
calculated  all  these  forks  wrongly) 

present  highest 

Hiplans's  Vocal  pitch  (meantone)  .... 

ft  If  (equal) 

Collard 

Erard 

Steinway  (in  England) 

Chappell 

8.  Military  Music. 

British  Army  regulation 

Kneller  Hall  Training  School  .... 


239 

323 
328 

329 
341 


358 
355 
357 


362 

336 
305 
329 
322 

282 

285 
320 
319 


235 
233 
272 

349 
357 
355 
358 


272 
323 

324 
321 

349 
357 
274 
284 

339 
359 
357 
362 


346 
350 


4299 

445-8 
447*3 
4477 

4506 


453-9 
455-1 

454-1 

454-7 

456-1 

449*2 
441-2 

447-5 
445-6 

449'7 
435-4 

4361 
445-5 
444-9 


423-7 
423-3 
4330 

452*5 
454-7 
454*1 
455*1 


433-0 
445-9 

4462 
445*5 

452*5 
454-7 
433-5 
436-0 

449-9 
455*3 
454*7 
4559 


451-9 
452-9 


f 


Digitized  by 


^.joogle 


5o8 


ADDITIONS  BY  THE   TRANSLATOR. 
Table  IL— CiiAssiFiKD  Index  to  Tabu  L-H^ontinued, 


APP.  XX. 


Divte 


Place 


Pitch 


1  Cents, 


1648 

PariB 

1766 

I* 

1854 

♦1 

I700P 

»» 

1832 

If 

1834 

»» 

1858 

II 

1859 

II 

II 

1 700c 

Lille 

1754 

II 

18000 

1854a 

II 

1859a 

It 

I8IOC 

Paris 

1636 

«i 

1700a 

LiUe 

1789 

Versailles 

I8I8 

Paris 

1840 

II 

1851a 

liiUe 

I85I 


I8II 
I8I9 
1822 
1824 
1829 

18340 
1836- 

1839 

1854a 

1855 

1856 

1858 

1823 
1856 

1820 
1823 
1836 

1859 

1838- 

54 

1859 


1836 

1812 


Paris 


Bordeaux 
LiUe 

Lyons 
Toulouse 


Paris 


IV.  France. 

1.  Standards. 

One  French  foot  pipe : 

Mersenne  c"447 

Dom  BMos  c"45o-5  . 

Delezenne,  c"446'4    . 
Pitch-pipe  at  Faculty  of  Sciences    . 
de  Prony's  proposal 
Marloye's  „  ... 

CavaiU^-Coll's  proposal  . 
Fr.  Com.  „         ... 

Diapason  Normal,  at  Conservatoire 

2.  Old  Forks, 

Mazingue's 

Fran^ois^B 

Cohen's 

Delezenne*8 

Marquis  d'AHgre's  .... 
Lemoine's 

3.  Church  Organs, 

Mersenne's  Um  de  chapelU 
L'Hospice  Comtesse 
Palace  Chapel,  fork  at  Conservatoire 
Tuileries  Chapel      .... 
St.  Denis  (Ctfvaill6-Coll)  . 

St.  Sauveur 

La  Madeleine  (restored)  . 

St.  Andr6 

4.  Concert  Organ, 
Festival  organ         .... 

5.  Opera, 

Grand  Opera : 

Boheibler 

Cagniard  de  la  Tour 

Fischer 

lowered  for  Branohu 
recovered  pitch 
orchestral  pitch 
Scheibler's  Petitbout 
Delezenne's  Leibner 

„  forks 

Lissajous  and  Ferrand 

Bodin 

Fr.  Com 

Italian  Opera : 

Fischer 

Bodin 

Op4ra  Comiquet  or  Feydeau. 

fork  at  Conservatoire 

Fischer 

Cagniard  de  la  Tour 
Provincial  Opera ; 

Fr.  Com 

Delezenne         .... 

Fr.  Com 

ij         .        .        .        ,        , 

6.  Concerts, 

Merscnne's  ton  de  chambre 
Conservatoire,  fork  there 


17 

31 

15 

178 

307 

262 

316 
280 
282 


66 
230 

255 
272 
297 
260 


534 
19 
117 
278 
317 
69 
129 
269 


327 


248 
276 
267 

243 
276 
300 
276 
304 

289 
335 
323 
331 

237 
329 

232 
250 
304 

312 
340 

331 
310 


726 
298 


Digitized  by  V^OOQIC 


SECT.  H.     THE   HISTORY   OF  MUSICAL  PITCH  IN  EUROPE. 
Table  II. — Clasbifibd  Index  to  Table  I,— continued. 


509 


Place 


Pitch 


Cent«t 


1822 

1834 

1815- 

1821 

1859 

1878 

1859 


Paris 


Toulouse 
Marseilles 


Paris 


Brunswiok 
Stuttgard 


Eutin 
Dresden 

N.  Germany 

Saxony 
Heidelberg 

»> 

Hamburg 

Palatinate 

Holstein 

Hamburg 

»i 

Holstein 

Saxony 

Strassburg 

Saxony 
Hamburg 
Dresden 

Hamburg 
Weimar 
Dresden 
Lubeck 

Hamburg 

Berlin 
Dresden 


Brunswiok 

Carlsruhe 

Gotha 

Weimar 

Stuttgard 


IV.  YtLkscE^amtinued, 

6.  Concerts — continued. 

Conservatoire,  Scheibler  I. 

..       II.     .        . 
„       III.  (Gand) 
de  la  Page 
Fr.  Com.  . 


I  7.  Pianofortes y  Spinets,  rfc. 

Mersenne's  spinet    .... 

'  Saaveur 

'  Pascal  Taskin  .... 
'  Piano  of  opera  .... 
I  WSlfePs 


y.  Gerxant. 

1.  Standards. 

Praetorius's  suitable  pitch 

Soheibler's  pitch  (reduced  to  59°  F.)  adopted  at  the 
Coilgress  of  Physicists 

2.  Old  Forks. 

F.  Anton  von  Weber's 


Eirsten's . 
Rummer's 


3.  Church  Organs  (in  order  of  date). 

Pretorius  (called  by  him  chamber  pitch)  highest 

recorded 

Halberstadt 

Schlick,  high  jpitch 

„       low  pitch 

St.  Catherine  (in  1879) 

Salomon  de  Cans 

Gliickstadt 

St.  Jacobi,  low  stop,  old  pitch         .        .        .        . 

„         high  stops,      „ 

Bendsburg 

Freiberg  Cathedral,  Silbermann      .        .        .        . 
Minster,  A.  Silbermann 


Dresden,  St.  Sophie         .        .        .        .        < 

Lehnert's  jTon^ti; 

Chained  fork  of  the  Boman  Catholic  Church 


Mattheson's  St.  Michaelis 
Topfer's  pipe   . 
Boman  Catholic  Church  . 
Cathedral,  old  organ 
St.  Jacobi,  modem  pitch 


5.  Opera  (arranged  by  towns). 

Fischer's  Pichler's  fork   .... 
Scheibler,  '  trustwortiiy  '  ... 


282 

303 
282 

324 
288 

327 


148 
163 
174 
242 

313 


237 
301 

236 


Naeke's  fork  of  Weber's  time 233 


Fr.  Com.  . 
Jehmlich's  fork 
Fr.  Com.  . 


4353 
4409 

435-2 
4462 
437-0 
447 


402-9 
4066 
4090 
425-5 
4433 


4242 
440-2 

424-1 


229 

4223 

239 

4249 

740 

5673 

541 

505-8 

535 

504-2 

33 

377-0 

454 

480-8 

119 

3964 

35° 

453-0 

184 

411-4 

484 

4892 

506 

495-5 

217 

419-5 

104 

393-2 

201 

415-5 

351 

455-2 

199 

4150 

169 

407-9 

237 

424-4 

212 

418-1 

465 

484-1 

500 

494-5 

289 

437*3 

307 

441-8 

233 

4232 

304 

441-0 

297 

439*4 

278 

443*5 

280 

435-0 

313 

4433 

319 

444-8 

312 

443 

Digitized  by  V^OOQ IC 


5IO 


ADDITIONS  BY  THE  TRANSLATOK. 
Table  £1.— Classified  Index  to  Table  I. — continued. 


APP.  XT- 


Date 


Place 


Pitch 


Oenta 


1859 
1869 


1879 

1859 

»» 
1869 

1776 


1859 


1720 
1 730c 
1 780c 


1 730c 
17800 


1845 

1856 
1857 

1859 
1869 

1839 


1781 
i860 


1802 
1858 


Munich 

Baden 

Wurtemberg 

Hamburg 


Leipzig 

Wurtemberg 

Leipzig 


Brealau 


The  Hague 


Rome 
[         Padua 


Florence 
Milan 
Turin 
Milan 

»» 
Naples 
Turin 


Bologna 


St.  Petersburg 


V.  Gebmany — contmued. 
Opera  (arranged  by  towns)— continued. 

Fr.  Com 

Sent  to  Society  of  Arts 

»f  If  .•••.. 

Similar  forks  sent  from  Berlin  and  Munich,  which 

had  adopted  French  pitch 
Opera  under  Erebs 

6.  Concerts. 

Old  orchestral  pitch 

Conservatoire  Fr.  Com 

Fr.  Com 

Oewandhaas,  sent  to  Society  of  Arts 

9.  Instruments. 


Marpurg  .... 
Naeke's  Schneider's  oboe 


VI.  Holland. 

3.  Church  Organs. 

The  old  celebrated  Church  organs  had  all  been  al- 
tered, and  I  have  not  succeeded  in  recovering 
their  ancient  pitch 

6.  Concerts. 
Fr.  Com 

VII.  Italy. 

I.  Standards. 
Pitch-pipes  of  Dr.  R.  Smith 


Mean  of  pitch-pipes  of  the  bell-fonndry  of  Col- 
bacchini 

2.  Old  Forks. 

From  Colbacchini's  low/" 

high/"    ,        ...        .        . 


5.  Opera. 


Marloye 


Fr.  Com. . 

La  Scala  (de  la  Fage) 
San  Carlo  (Guillaume) 
Fr.  Com. . 


6.  Concerts. 
Liceo  Musicale  (Society  of  Arts) 

7.  Pianofortes. 
Tadolini's  fork         .        .        .        . 

VIII.  Russia. 

3.  Church  Organs. 
Euler 


332 
278 
288 


331 


320 
334 
3*9 
332 


196 
191 


4481 
434-5 
436-9 

448-0 


445-0  I 

448-8  I 
444-8 

448-2  I 


414-4 
413-3 


334 

114 
241 


152 
230 


287 
326 
299 
349 
345 
319 
319 


312 


243 


211 
224 


284 
345 


Fr.  Com.  (French  pitch  was  afterwards  adopted) 

Digitized  by  V^OOQIC 


446-2 


395-2 
425-2 


403-9 
422*6 


436-7 
446*6 

439-4 
450*3 

45«7 
444-9 
444*8 


4431   ! 
425-8 


4iS-o 
421*2 


436-0 
4S'-5 


iECT.  H.     THE  HISTORY  OF  MUSICAL  PITCH  IN  EUROPE. 
Table  II. — Classitibd  Index  to  Table  I. — conliniied. 


5^1 


Date 


Place 


Pitch 


Cents 


1785 
1858 


1868 
1880 


1879 
1880 


Seville 
Madrid 


New  York 

Boston 

New  York 

It 

Boston 

Cincinnati 
New  York 


IX.  Spain. 


3.  Church  Organs. 


T.  Bosch's  organ 
Ton  de  Ghapelle 


5.  Opera. 
Theatre  (French  pitch  adopted  in  1879)  • 

X.  United  States  of  America. 

£.  S.  Bitohie's  standard,  and  Mason  &  Hamlin's 

French  pitch 

Church  of  Immaculate  Conception  . 
Hutchings,  Plaisted  &  Co.,  *  low  organ  pitch ' 
NichoPs  Fork,  Germania  orchestra . 
Music  Hall  organ  (from  1863  to  1871  at  French 

pitch) 

Organ  tuned  to  Thomas's  orchestra 

Steinway's  American  pitch,  from  a  fork  furnished 

by  Steinway 

Steinway's,  from  a  fork  furnished  by  B.  Spice 
Highest  New  York  pitch,  from  a  fork  furnished  by 

B.  Spice 


218 

218 


318 


283 
315 
316 
333 

342 
362 

366 
369 

380 


419-6 
•419-6 


444*5 


435*9 
443*9 
444*2 
448-5 

450*9 
456-1 

457*2 
4580 

460*8 


Conclusions. 

Art.  6.  The  two  preceding  tables  contain  the  &icts  of  the  histoiy  of  musical 
dtch  in  Europe  since  1361,  the  date  of  the  Halberstadt  organ,  that  is  for  500  years, 
o  fiftr  as  I  have  been,  able  to  collect  information,  and  I  have  been  fortunate  enough 
o  bring  together  such  an  amount  of  historical  evidence  that  probably  no  new  IT 
acts  could  be  ascertained  which  would  materially  change  the  conclusions  to  which 
have  been  led.    These  are  very  briefly  as  follows. 

Art.  7.  The  organ  was  originally  a  mere  collection  of  pitch-pipes,  each  with  a 
ixed  tone,  to  steady  the  voice  of  the  singers  of  ecclesiastical  chants,  replacing  the 
ingle  pitch-pipe  with  a  movable  piston  or  some  instrument  like  the  flageolet 
whistle)  and  oboe,  which  subsequently  gave  rise  to  the  two  distinct  series  of  flue  and 
eed  pipes.  But  when  thus  collected  it  was  necessary  to  fix  a  pitch.  The  guiding 
»rinciples  were  the  compass  of  the  male  voice,  the  rules  of  ecclesiastical  song,  the 
lase  of  the  performer,  to  avoid  introducing  chromatics  as  much  as  possible  (Schlick), 
knd  the  standard  measure  or  foot  rule  of  the  country.  The  latter  suggested  a 
rhole  number  of  feet  for  the  length  of  the  standard  pipe,  generally  four  feet,  about 
he  lowest  note  of  the  tenor  voice,  and  the  question  thus  rose  what  note  should 
his  tone  represent  ?  Here  the  answer  came  from  ecclesiastical  use, — either  F  or 
.  Schhck  recommends  both,  thus  giving  pitches  for  any  given  note  a  whole 
rourth  apart.  Bchlick's  high  pitch,  arising  from  giving  a  6^-foot  Rhenish  pipe  ^ 
o  c,  made  a^5o4'2.  (All  pitches  named  should  be  referred  to  in  Table  i.)  His 
ow  pitch  arising  from  giving  the  same  pipe  to  F,  made  a'377.  These  are  a  Mean- 
one  Fourth  apart. 


Art.  8. — The  foot  had  very  different  lengths 
Q  different  countries.  If  we  suppose  the '  scale  * 
or  ratio  of  diameter  to  length  of  pipe)  and  the 
orce  of  wind  to  remain  the  same  (both  in  fact 
aried  much),  then  the  influence  of  the  length 
•f  the  foot  on  the  pitch  of  the  organ,  suppos- 
og  the  four-foot  or  one-foot  pipe  to  be  given 
o  the  same  note,  may  be  appreciated  from 
he  table  on  p.  512a.  In  this  we  see  a  differ- 
nce  of  more  than  a  Tone,  nearly  a  minor 
niird,  between  the  pitch  of  a  i-foot  pipe  in 
l*rance  and  in  Saxony.  The  difference  be- 
ween  the  pitches  of  pipes  of  the  lengths  of 


the  English  foot  and  French  foot  is  more  than 
an  equal  Semitone.  Hence  probably  it  hap- 
pened that  the  lowest  French  pitch  measured, 
a'374'2,  is  a  Semitone  flatter  than  the  lowest 
English  pitch  measured  a'395*2.  Length  of 
foot  alone  would  therefore  account  for  great 
variety  of  organ-pitch,  to  which  we  must  add 
force  of  wind  (see  the  notes  on  experimental 
English  I -foot  pipes,  p.  506c)  and  different 
methods  of  voicing.  The  low  pitches  were  (and 
still  are  on  old  organs)  prevalent  in  France 
and  Spain,  the  high  pitches  were  at  home  in 
North  Germany  (see  Table  II.). 


Digitized  by  VjOOQIC 


5" 


ADDITIONS  BY  THE  TRANSLATOR. 


AFP.  XX. 


Names  of  Feet 

Intcrral 

Long  old  French  foot,  or  pied  deroi  . 

Long  Austrian  foot 

Long  German,  or  Bhenish  foot    . 

English  foot 

Old  Niirnberg  foot 

Old  Augsburg  foot       .        ^        ^        .        . 
Old  Roman  foot  (medieval) 

Bavarian  foot 

Short  Hamburg  and  Danish  foot 
Short  Brunswick  and  Frankfurt  foot  . 
Short  Saxon  foot 

mm. 
325 
316 
314 
305 
304 
296 
295 
292 
286 
28S 
283 

oenta 
0 

49 
60 
109 
116 
162 
168 

185 
221 
227 
239 

Art.  9. — The  solo  instmiments  were  tuned  very  variously.  But  it  became  the 
^  custom  to  have  a  band  to  play  with  the  organ,  and  the  princes  and  petty  dukes  used 
the  same  bands  to  play  in  their  private  apartments  or  *  chamber.*  The  very  high 
and  very  low  pitch  were  generaUy  found  unsuitable  for  non-ecclesiastical  music. 
Hence  the  instruments  usually  adopted  a  pitch  lower  than  the  high  and  higher  than 
the  low,  and  this  was  called  *  chamber  pitch,'  the  other  being  distir^ruighed  as 
'  church  pitch.'  But  the  same  instruments  had  also  to  play  with  &e  organ. 
Hence  the  difference  had  to  be  a  definite  number  of  degrees  of  the  scale,  a  Semi- 
tone, a  Tone,  or  a  minor  Third.  See  a'407-9,  and  especially  a'4ii'4,  which  com- 
pare with  a'480'8,  and  a'484-i  respectively.  This  was,  however,  not  always  the 
case,  for  the  very  high  church  pitch,  a'5037  had  a  still  higher  chamber  pitch 
a'563-i. 

Art.  10. — But  this  great  variety  occasioned  much  trouble,  and  the  chamber 
pitch  below  the  high  and  above  the  low  church  pitch  seems  to  have  suggested 
PrsBtorius's  *  suitable  pitch '  of  a'424-2  in  1619.  This  was  in  fact  a  *  mean  pitch,' 
and  as  such  rapidly  found  such  favour  that  it  spread  over  all  Europe  and,  with 
H  insignificant  varieties  (from  a' 41$  to  a'428'7  at  the  extremes,  an  interval  of  54 
cents,  or  a  quarter  of  a  Tone),  prevailed  for  two  centuries.  Handers  own  fork, 
a'422'5  in  1 75 1,  quite  a  common  pitch  at  the  time,  and  the  London  Philharmonic 
fork,  a'423*3  from  its  foundation  to  1820,  are  conspicuous  examples,  but  an  inspec- 
tion of  the  numerous  pitches  cited  in  Table  I.  sect.  4  (pp-  495^7),  will  prove  tiie 
fact  beyond  doubt. 

Art.  II. — As  this  was  the  period  of  the  great  musical  masters,  and  as  their 
music  is  still  sung,  and  sung  frequently,  it  is  a  great  pity  that  the  pitch  should 
have  been  raised,  and  that  Handel,  Haydn,  Mozart,  Beethoven,  and  Weber,  for 
example,  should  be  sung  at  a  pitch  more  than  a  Semitone  higher  than  they  in- 
tended. The  high  pitch  strains  the  voices  and  hence  deteriorates  from  the  effect 
of  the  music,  when  applied  to  compositions  not  intended  for  it.  Of  course  for 
music  written  for  a  high  pitch  the  compass  of  the  human  voice  is  properly  studied 
(see  App.  XX.  sect.  N.  No.  i),  and  so  much  music  has  in  the  last  fifty  years  been 
written  for  a  high  pitch,  that  to  perform  both  properly  two  sets  of  instruments 
would  be  required.  Two  sets  are  actually  in  use  at  Dresden,  one  for  the  theatre 
If  a'439'4,  and  one  for  the  Roman  Cathohc  Church  having  a'415,  diff^erence  98  cents, 
or  about  a  Semitone. 

Art.  12. — The  rise  in  pitch  began  at  the  great  Congress  of  Vienna,  181 4,  when 
the  Emperor  of  Eussia  presented  new  and  sharper  wind  instruments  to  an  Austrian 
regiment  of  which  he  was  colonel.  The  band  of  this  regiment  became  noted  for 
the  brilhancy  of  its  tones.  In  1820  another  Austrian  regiment  received  even 
sharper  instruments,  and  as  the  theatres  were  greatly  dependent  upon  the  bands 
of  the  home  regiments,  they  were  obliged  to  adopt  their  pitch.  Gradually  at 
Vienna,  pitch  rose  from  a'42i*6  (Mozart's  pitch)  to  a'456'i,  that  is,  136  cents,  or 
nearly  three-quarters  of  a  Tone.  The  mania  spread  throughout  Europe,  but  at 
very  different  rates.  The  pitch  reached  a'448  at  the  Paris  Opera  in  1858,  and  the 
musical  world  took  fright. 

Art.  13. — The  Emperor  of  the  French  appointed  a  commission  to  select  a  pitch, 
and  this  determined  on  a'435,  but  made  a  fork  called  Diapason  normal,  now 
found  to  be  a'435'4,  which  is  preserved  at  the  Mus^e  du  Conservatoire,  and  is  the 
only  standard  pitch  in  the  world.   This  pitch  was  widely  adopted,  but  it  i{5  56  cent^, 

Digitized  by  VjOOQlC 


SECT.  H.     THE  HISTORY   OF  MUSICAL  PITCH  IN  EUROPE.  513 

or  over  a  quarter  of  a  Tone,  sharper  than  Mozart's  pitch,  although  it  was  80  cents, 
fully  three-quarters  of  a  Semitone,  flatter  than  the  old  Vienna  sharp  pitch  a'  456*1, 
and  49  cents,  or  a  quarter  of  a  Tone,  flatter  than  the  then  French  opera  pitch 
a'  448.  This  pitch  had  been  reached  independently  in  many  places,  and  the  French 
commission  had  been  twitted  at  taking  a  Carlsruhe  pitch.  But  it  is  not  generally 
known  that  Sir  George  Smart's  pitch  a!  433,  adopted  with  much  hesitation  for  the 
London  Philharmonic  Society  about  1820,  and  extensively  sold  in  London  as  the 
'  London  Philharmonic  *  for  many  years  before  the  French  Commission  of  1859, 
was  in  fact  an  anticipation  of  the  French  piitch.  Both  were  compromises,  a  partial 
yielding  to  the  new  without  entirely  disregarding  the  old.  The  pitches  a'  430  to 
a'  436-9,  therefore  (interval  28  cents,  or  about  \  Tone),  forming  Table  I.  Sect.  5, 
pp.  497-8,  are  termed  the  *  compromise  pitch.'  As  instruments  exist  for  this 
pitch  it  is  the  only  one  that  has  a  chance  of  being  used  beside  the  present 
sharp  pitch  of  England.  Several  attempts  have  been  made  to  restore  it«  notably 
at  Covent  Garden  Opera  in  1880.  But  the  expense  of  new  instruments  for  a 
band,  about  i,oooZ.,  renders  any  alteration  extremely  difficult  to  carry  out.  The  ^ 
tendency  in  England  has  been  to  sharpen,  and  our  orchestral  and  pianoforte  pitch 
is  now  from  a'  449*7  to  a'  454*7,  a  difference  of  only  19  cents,  not  quite  a  comma. 
In  the  United  States,  however,  the  pitch  has  reached  a'  460*8,  that  is  23  cents,  or 
about  a  comma  more.  In  Germany  the  compromise  pitch  adopted  was  a'  440*2 
as  proposed  by  Scheibler,  and  it  is  curious  that  the  standard  pipes  of  the  English 
church  organ  builders  vary  from  al  436  to  a'  445*2,  36  cents,  but  are  mostly 
between  440  and  441  '7,  an  interval  of  only  7  cents.  The  concert  organs,  of  course, 
follow  orchestral  pitch.     (See  Postscript,  p.  555.) 

Art.  14. — In  England  the  pitch  of  organs  varied  with  the  note  on  which  the 
four-foot  or  one-foot  pipe  was  placed.  We  have  only  one  record  that  the  one- 
foot  pipe  was  placed  on  c"  giving  a'  395*2,  whereas  the  same  pipe  made  to  give  b' 
produced  a'  423,  the  mean  pitch,  which  so  long  prevailed.  Put  on  b^}  it  produced 
a^  442,  which  as  a'  441*7  was  Bernard  Schmidt's  low  pitch,  and  is  still  the  pitch 
of  Mr.  T.  Hill,  the  organ-builder.  Placed  on  a'  it  gave  a'  472*9,  which  as  a'  474*1 
was  the  highest  church  pitch  used  in  England,  just  a  Tone  above  mean  pitch. 
(See  p.  505c,  III.  3,  for  details.)  ^ 

Art.  15. — If  we  look  into  the  secrets  of  the  rise  of  pitch  we  find  it  always  con- 
nected with  wind  instruments.  The  first  rise  was  from  a  military  band,  and  the  wind 
and  the  brass  have  constantly  rebelled  against  a  low  pitch.  The  singers  have  not 
prevailed  against  them  except  for  a  very  short  time.  The  great  violin  school  of 
Oremona  in  Italy  lived  in  the  time  of  mean  pitch  with  a  higher  chamber  pitch, 
and  the  resonance  of  the  boxes  of  their  violins  seems  to  shew  traces  of  the  action 
of  both  pitches  (suprd.,  p.  87,  note  *),  but  their  great  object  was  to  insure  tolerable 
uniformity  of  reinforcement,  and  hence  they  are  a  treasure  for  aU  time. 

Art.  16. — The  only  possible  conclusion  seems  to  be  that  to  sing  music  written 
for  pitches  different  from  our  own,  we  must  either  transpose  a  Semitone  (always  a 
difficulty,  and  for  some  instruments  an  impossibility)  or  adopt  a  new  compromise 
pitch,  the  French,  already  once  firmly  rooted  in  England  as  Sir  George  Smart's,  and 
standing  half-way  between  the  extremes.  On  the  continent,  as  formerly  shewn  in 
France,  and  quite  recently  in  Belgium  and  Italy,  the  government  has  a  certain 
power  in  fixing  musical  pitch,  by  refusing  to  subsidise  conservatories  and  theatres 
which  do  not  adopt  the  pitch  ordered,  and  commanding  the  regimental  bands  to  % 
make  the  change.  But  beyond  this  their  power  does  not  extend,  and  the  various 
regulations  which  have  been  made  in  the  two  countries  last  named  shew  the 
great  difficulties  that  have  to  be  overcome  in  introducing  a  new  pitch  even  within 
the  area  under  government  control.  In  England,  however,  there  are  no  subsidised 
operas  or  musical  conservatories,  and  even  the  instruments  of  the  military  bands 
are  not  provided  by  government.  Hence  the  change  must  be  left  to  the  gradual 
action  of  musical  feeling.  We  have  already  changed  in  England  almost  imper- 
ceptibly. The  raising  of  English  pitch  from  Sir  George  Smart's  a'  433  was  to  a 
great  extent  due  to  the  individual  action  of  the  late  Sir  Michael  Costa  while  con- 
ductor of  the  Philharmonic  concerts  1846-45  (mean  a'  452*5,  extreme  a'  454*7), 
to  whose  insistence  is  also  due  the  high  pitch  of  the  Albert  Hall  concert  organ, 
a'  453*9.  Perhaps  a  similar  energetic  conductor  will  arise  to  turn  the  tide  of 
musical  opinion  in  the  opposite  direction. 


Digitized  by 


GcJogle 


5M 


ADDITIONS  BY   THE   TRANSLATOR. 


APP.  XI. 


SECTION   K. 

NON-HARUONIC   SCALES. 


(See  Notes,  pp.  7i»  95.  237.  253»  255,  257,  258,  264,  272.) 


Art. 
I.  iDtroduotion,  p.  514. 
n.  Table  of  Non-Harmonic  Scales,  p.  514. 
III.  Annotations  to  the  Table,  p.  519. 


IV.  How  these  Divisions  of  the  OetaTe  maj 
have  arisen,  p.  522. 
V.  Besalts  of  the  inquiry,  p.  524. 


I.  Inteoduction. 

For  particulars  of  my  researches  into  non-harmonic  scales,  see  my  two  papers, 
first :  *  Tonometrical  Observations  on  some  existing  non-harmonic  Scales  *  (Proc. 
of  the  B.  Society  for  Nov.  20, 1884,  vol.  xxxvii.  p.  368)  and  second  *  On  the  Musical 
Scales  of  Various  Nations  *  (Journal  of  the  Society  of  Arts  for  March  27,  1885, 
m  vol.  xxxiii.  p.  485),  in  both  of  which  I  was  most  materially  assisted  by  Mr.  Alfred 
James  Hiplans  of  Messrs.  J.  Broad  wood  and  Sons*. 

Properly  speaking  there  is  only  one  harmonic  scale,  that  is,  a  scale  which 
allows  the  musician  to  produce  chords  without  beats,  and  therefore  has  notes  with 
pitch  numbers  composed  of  products  and  multiples  of  the  powers  of  2,  3,  5,  7,  17, 
as  shewn  in  Sect.  E.  But  the  term  harmonic  may  be  extended  to  all  tempered 
imitations  of  such  scales  as  are  not  worse  than  equal  intonation.  If  we  did  not 
extend  the  use  of  the  term  thus  far,  we  should  find  absolutely  no  harmonic  scale  in 
practical  use,  except  by  the  Tonic  Sol-faists  when  unaccompanied  (App.  XVIII.) 
Even  with  this  extension  of  meaning,  non-harmonic  scales  are  greatly  more 
numerous  than  harmonic.  Harmony  was  a  European  discovery  of  a  few  centmiei 
back,  and  it  has  not  penetrated  beyond  Europe  and  its  colonies. 

scale  is  then  found  by  subtraction.  The 
number  of  the  note  in  the  scale  is  usually 
placed  at  the  top,  so  that  the  eye  can,  at  a 
glance,  compare  the  different  usages.  The 
ratios  represented  by  these  cents  may  g«ii»- 
rally  be  found  from  the  table  in  Sect.  D. 
Each  scale  is  numbered,  and  in  the  annoCs- 
tions  immediately  following  the  table,  sevecsl 
particulars  are  given.  It  was  not,  howeter, 
possible  to  include  every  case  in  this  arrange- 
ment. The  complete  ancient  and  medienl 
Arabian  lute,  Bab&b,  and  Tambour  scales,  and 
the  complete  Indian  scales  both  in  the  old  and 
modem  form,  and  some  others  are  therefoR 
differently  ordered,  preserving,  however,  tbe 
expression  of  notes  by  cents  as  above  ei* 
plained.    See  Nos.  66  to  75. 


In  order  to  obtain  a  bird's-eye  glance  over 
the  scales  given  theoretically  by  ancient  Greek 
writers  (as  interpreted  in  the  text),  by  ancient 
m  and  medieval  Arabic  writers  (as  interpreted 
by  Professor  Land);  by  modem  Arabic 
theorists  (as  reported  by  Eli  Smith);  by 
Indian  musicians  (as  reported  by  Bajah 
Sourindro  Mohun  Tagore) ;  and  those  which 
I  have  deduced  from  Javese,  Chinese,  and 
Japanese  instruments,  with  those  of  other 
countries,  examined  by  Mr.  Hipkins  and  my- 
self, I  have  constracted  the  following  table. 
The  scale  is  represented  by  the  numbers 
of  cents  in  the  interval  by  which  any  one 
of  its  notes  is  sharper  than  the  lowest  note, 
and  is  generally  confined  to  one  octave. 
The  interval  between  any  two  notes  in  the 


II  Table  of  Non-Habmonic  Scales. 


Old  Oreek  Tetrachords. 


1.  Olympos    . 

2.  Old  Chromatic  . 

3.  Diatonic 

4.  IHdymns    . 

5.  Doric 

6.  Phrygian   . 

7.  Lydian 

8.  Helmholtz 

9.  Soft  Diatonic     . 

10.  Ptolemy's  equal  diatonic 
XI.  Enharmonic 


I. 

II. 

in. 

IV. 

0 

112 



4^ 

0 

112 

182 

498 

0 

112 

316 

«! 

0 

112 

294 

<9! 

0 

90 

294 

498 

0 

182 

316 

498 

0 

182 

386 

498 

0 

112 

386 

49S 

0 

85 

267 

498    , 

0 

151 

316 

498    ' 

0 

55 

113 

498 

Greek  Tetrachords  after  Al  Fa/rabi  reported  by  Prof.  Land. 

I.  Oenus  molle,  ordinatttm. 
a,  continuum : — 

12.  laxum I      o       I         386         I         441 

13.  mediocre \       ^  3'^         '         4^5 

14-  acre 1       o  267         j         386         , 


498 
498 
498 


Digitized  by  VjOOQlC 


BCT.  K. 


NON-HARMONIC   SCALES. 
II.  Tabls  of  Non-Habmonio  Scales — continued. 


515 


Greek  Tetrachorde  after  Al  Fardbi  reported  by  Prof.  Land—  continaed. 


I.  Genua  molley  ordinatum — oont. 
b.  non  oontinuum. 

15.  lazum  (enharmonic) . 

16.  mediocre  (soft  chromatic) 

17.  acre  (syntonically  chromatic)    . 

II.  Genua  forte. 

a.  duplicatom: — 

18.  primum 

19.  secundum 

2a  tertinm 

6.  conjonctnm: — 

21.  primnm  (entonically  diatonic) 

22.  seonndum  (syntonically  diatonic) 

23.  tertinm  (equally  diatonic) 

6.  disjunctum:— 

24.  primum  (soft  diatonic) 


o 
o 
a 


o 
o 
o 

o 
o 
o 


n. 


386 
316 
267 


231 
204 
182 

231 
204 
182 

231 


TIL 


460 

435 
418 


462 
408 
365 

435 
386 

347 
413 


IV. 


Most  Ancient  Form  of  Oreek  Scales  with  7  Tones  and  Octave, 

VI.      viL     vin. 


25.  Lydian 

26.  Phrygian  . 

27.  Doric 

28.  Hypolydian 

29.  Hypophxygian  (Ionic) 

30.  Hypodoric  (Eolic) 

31.  Mixolydian 


I. 

11. 

III. 

IV. 

V. 

0 

182 

386 

498 

702 

0 

182 

316 

498 

702 

0 

90 

294 

498 

70a 

0 

204 

386 

590 

702 

0 

204 

386 

498 

702 

0 

204 

294 

498 

702 

0 

112 

294 

498 

610 

884 
884 
792 
884 
884 
792 
814 


Later  Oreek  Scales  toith  Pythagorean  Intonation, 


32.  Lydian 

33.  Hypophrygian  (Ionic) 

34.  Phrygian  . 

35.  Eolio 

36.  Doric  (same  as  No.  27) 

37.  Siixolydian 

38.  Syntonolydian  . 


0 

204 

408 

498 

702 

906 

IIIO 

0 

204 

408 

498 

702 

906 

996 

0 

204 

294 

498 

702 

906 

996 

0 

204 

294 

498 

702 

792 

996 

0 

90 

294 

498 

702 

792 

996 

0 

90 

294 

498 

588 

792 

996 

0 

204 

408 

612 

702 

906 

IIIO 

1200 
1200 

I203 
1200 
1200 
1200 
1200 


Al  Farahi's  Oreek  Scales  as  reported  by  Prof.  Land, 


39.  Qenus  conjunotum  medium 

40.  Genus  duplicatum  medium,  or  dito- 

num  (same  as  No.  38)     . 

41.  Qenxxs  conjunctum  primum 

42.  Genus  forte  duplicatum  primum 

43.  Genus  conjunotum  tertium,  or  forte 

flsquatum 

44.  Qenus  forte  disjunctum  primum 

45.  Genus  non  continuum  acre 

46.  Genus  non  continuum  mediocre 

47.  Gbnus  non  oontinuum  laxum    . 

48.  Genus  ohromaticum  forte  . 

49.  Genus  ohromaticum  moUissimum 

50.  Genus  moUissimum  ordinantium 


0 

204 

408 

590 

702 

0 

204 

408 

612 

702 

0 

204 

435 

639 

702 

0 

204 

435 

666 

702 

0 

204 

386 

551 

702 

0 

204 

435 

6i7 

702 

0 

204 

471 

629 

702 

0 

204 

520 

639 

702 

0 

204 

590 

664 

702 

0 

204 

471 

690 

702 

0 

204 

520 

613 

702 

0 

204 

590 

647 

702 

906  ,  1088  ;  1200 


906 
933 
933 

884 

933 
969 
1018 
1088 
969 
1018 
1088 


I  no  1 200 

II37  I  1200 

1 164  I  1200 

i 

1049  I  1200 

III5  I  1200 

I 120  I  I20O 


"37 
1 162 
1088 

nil 
1 145 


1200 
1200 
1200 
1200 
1200 


H 


Arabic  and  Persian  Scales  as  reported  by  Prof.  Land. 

51.  Zalzal.  see  No.  66      .        .        .        .  |    o    |  204  |  355  |  498  |  702  |    853  1     996  |   1200 

Highland  Bagpipe  made  by  Macdonald  of  Edinburgh. 

52.  Observed 1  o  |  197  |  341  I  495  |  703  I  853  1  1009  |  1200 

Modem  Arabic  Scale  as  reported  by  Eli  Smith, 

53.  Meshaqah,  theoretical       .  .  ;    o       200  '  350  |  500  |  700  [     850  |  1000  |  1200  \ 

Digitized  by  VjOQQ IC 


Si6 


ADDITIONS  BY  THE   TEANSLATOE. 
II.  Tablx  of  Kon-Habuonxc  QckLKB—eontinuid, 


APP.  XI. 


H 


Arabic  Medieval  Scales  as  reported  by  Prof.  Land  with  7  Tones  and  Octave. 

I.         II.    I   III.  i    IV.        V.         VI.        Vn.       VHL 


54.  *Oohaq  (same  as  No.  33) 

55.  Nawa  (same  as  No.  34) 

56.  BoasUik  (same  as  No.  37) 

57.  Bast  . 

58.  Zenkonleh 

59.  Bahawi 

60.  Hhosaini  . 

61.  Hhidjazi    . 


II. 

III.  1 

204 

408 

204 

294 

90 

294 

204 

3«4 

204 

380 

180 

384 

180 

294 

180 

294 

IV. 

498 

V. 

702 

498 

702 

498 

588 

498 

702 

498 

678 

498 

678 

498 

678 

498 

678 

906 
906 

792 
882 
882 
792 
906 
882 


996 
996 

996 

996 
996 
996 
996 
996 


1200 

1200 

1200 

1200 

1200 

1200  ; 

1200 


1200 


Arabic  Medieval  Scales  with  8  Tones  and  Octave. 


62.  'Iraq 

63.  Isfahan 

64.  Zirafkend 

65.  Boozoork  , 


II. 


180 
180 
180 
180 


III. 


384 
384 
294 

384 


IV. 


498  678 


498 
498 
498 


702 
678 
678 


VI. 


882 
882 
792 

702 


VII.  vin.  IX. 


996  1 176 
996  1176 
882  I10861200 
906  ,10861200 


1200 
1200 


66.  Earlier  Notes  on  the  Arabic  Lute  as  reported  by  Prof.  Land. 

*^^*  First,  Sxoond,  Ac,  refer  to  the  strings.    The  notes  are  named  from  the  fingers — ^indez, 
middle,  ring,  little — by  which  they  were  played. 


Notes 


C 


D 


Fb 
E 


F 
Gb 


G 

Ab 


Bbb 
A 


Bb 
Cb 


First  OctaTe 


Fibst:  open 
ancient  near  index 

Persian  near  index 

Zalzal's  near  index 

index 

ancient  middle  . 

Persian  middle  . 

Zalzal's  middle 


Second  OctaTe 


Cents 


Oct.] 


Third  :  index  , 
ancient  middle  . 
Persian  middle 


Zalxal's  middle 

ring  .... 
little  a.  Fourth:  open 


nng 


little  «  Second  :  open   ^     . 
Second  :  ancient  near  index 


Persian  near  index 

ZalzaPs  near  index 

index 

ancient  middle  . 

Persian  middle  . 

Zalzal*s  middle 


nng 


little  =  Third:  open  . 
Third  :  ancient  near  index 


Persian  near  index 

Zalzal*s  near  index 
index 


Fourth  :  ancient  near  index 


Persian  near  index 

Zalzal's  near  index 

index 

ancient  middle  . 

Persian  middle  . 


2Uzal*s  middle 

ring   .... 
little  aFiTTH:  open  . 


Fifth  :  ancient  near  index 


o 
90 

145 

168 
204 
294 
303 
355 

408 


498 
588 


Oct.  a 

IMO-t- 


O 
90 

99 
151 

204 
294 


384 

439 
462 

498 
588 

597 


643  - 


Persian  near  index 
Zalzal's  near  index 


Fifth  :  ancient  middle 
Persian  middle  . 


Zalzal's  middle 
ring  . 


666 

702 

792 
801 

853 
906 


996 
1086 

1 141 

1 164 
1200 


649 

702 
792 


882 

937 
960 

1086 
1095 

1147 


Digitized  by  V^OOQIC 


SECT.  K. 


NON-HARMONIC  SCALES. 
II.  Table  of  Non-Habmonio  BoAiMa^~<ontinued. 


517 


67.  Medieval  Arabic  Scales  as  reported  by  Prof.  Land. 
*^*  Names  of  stringB  as  in  No.  66,  names  of  notes  as  altered  by  the  Arabic  medieval  writers. 


Kt^ 

Notes 

I 

c 

2 

Db 

3 

^t>b 

4 

D 

5 

Eb 

6 

Fb 

7 

E 

8 

F 

9 

Ob 

10 

Abb 

II 

G 

12 

Ab 

13 

Bbb 

14 

A 

15 

Bb 

16 

Cb 

17 

Dbb 

1' 

e 

First  Ootare 


First:  open 
remnant 
near    . 
index  . 
Persian 
Zalzal 
ring    . 


UtU«  . 
S]sooin>:  remnant 
near  . 
index  . 
Persian 
Zalzal 
ring    . 


Uttle  . 
Thibd:  remnant 
near   . 
index 


Cents 


Second  Octaye 


Thibd:  index 

Persian 

Zalzal 

ring    • 

Uttle  .    . 
FouBTH :  renmant 


near  {  =  Obb) 
index  . 
Persian 
Zalzal 
ring  . 
Uttle  . 
Fifth  :  remnant 


near(»Cbl>) 
index  . 
Persian 
Zalzal 
ring    . 


68.  Northern  Tambour,  or  that  of  Khorassan,  as  reported  by  Prof  Land, 

Co.  Db  90,  Ebb  180,  ♦D204,  £b294.  Fb384,  £7408,  ♦F498,  Gb  588,  ^«6i2,  ♦G702, 
til b  792,  GS816,  i4  9o6,  £b996,  filQ  1020,  JB  iiio,  '^ci200,  691224,  c9  13141  *^  1404 
cents. 

*  Fixed  tones.  t  Aitxiliary  tones. 


Babdb  or  2'Stringed  viol,  after  Prof,  Land, 


69.  first I  o 

70.  second jo 

71.  third I  o 


204 

316 

408 

520 

590 

632 

724 

204 

316 

408 

590 

612 

724 

816 

204 

316 

408 

590 

794 

906 

998 

906 
998 

XI80 


Southern  Tamboury  or  that  of  Bagdad^  as  reported  by  Prof  Land, 
72.  theoretical      .        .        .        .  |  o  |  44  |  89  |  135  |  182  |  231   |  275  |  320  |  366  |  413  |  462 

Indian  Chromatic  Scale, 

*«*  Arranged  according  to  the  older  and  more  modem  division  as  inferred  from  indications 
by  Bajah  Sourindro  Mohan  Tagore. 


Degrees 
Notes 

73.  Old    . 

74.  New  . 


Dbb 

49 


£bb  \i^ 


36411325*386 
259  1316  37+ 


9  ;  zo'  XI I  Z3  I  13  '14 

Bt\F\  —  \Ft\F%t     V 

442  498  549J60O  ,   651    j7M 

4351498  543'589    637  I685 


15   16 

17  18    19 

30 

31 

AbbAb 

-    AiB.b 

80 

B 

753  804 
736  787 

855  9o6;966| 
841,896,  95a 

xoa7i 

lOII 

1088 
1070 

33 
Bt 
"44 
"35 


Indian  Semdtonic  Scale  as  inferred  from  Measurement  of  a  Madras  Vina, 

75.  First  Octave.        .        .  I      ol    89:  178!  2691  373I  4751  S96|  6841  781   8791  99610811199 
Second  Octave      •        .  I1199I1280I1376I1466I156711681I177611891I19842090J2187I2298I2398 

♦^*  The  Indian  partial  scales  enumerated  by  Rajah  S.  M.  Tagore,  as  made  up  from  the  19 
inotes  in  Nos.  73  or  74,  32  of  them  with  7  notes,  112  vdth  6  notes,  and  160  with  5  notes  each, 
are  not  given  because  he  does  not  distinguish  the  minor  variations  of  one  degree. 


Indian  Partial  Scales  as  played  by  Bajah  Bdm  Pdl  Singh, 


76.  First 

77.  Fourth 

78.  Second 

79.  Third 

80.  Fifth 


I. 
0 

II. 

III. 

IV.    1     Y. 

VI. 

VII. 

1074 

183 

342 

533 

68s 

0 

174 

350 

477 

697 

908 

1079 

0 

183 

271 

534 

686 

872 

983 

0 

III 

314 

534 

686 

828 

1017 

0 

90 

366 

493 

707 

781 

1080 

VIII. 


1230 
II8I 
1232 
IJ98 
1087 


IT 


Digitized  by  V^OOQIC 


5i8 


ADDITIONS  BY  THE   TRANSLATOR. 


APP.  XX. 


II.  Tablb  op  Non-Harmonic  Scales — conHnued, 


Various  Wood  Harmonicons. 


8i.  Balafong  from  Patna 

82.  Balafong  from  Singapore 

83.  Patala  from  Btirmah 

84.  Balafong  from  the  same  . 

85.  Banat  from  Siam,    See  p.  556 

86.  Balafong  from  Western  Africa 

87.  Gen.    Pitt-Biyers'B   Balaiong  from 

the  same 


n. 

III. 

lY. 

V. 
672 

VI. 

VII. 

VllL 

187 

356 

526 

856 

985 

1222 

169 

350 

543 

709 

894 

1040 

1205 

176 

350 

533 

707 

899 

1053 

1246 

114 

350 

550 

687 

838 

1032 

1 196 

129 

277 

508 

726 

771 

1029 

1254 

152 

287 

533 

724 

890 

1039 

1200 

195 

28g 

513 

686 

796 

1008 

1209 

Pbntatonic  Scales. 
The  Black  Digitals  of  a  Pianoforte. 


88.  beginning  with  Ct  . 
90.  n  „     FU  . 

91.  ..  M  O^      . 

92.  »  „     AU  . 


I. 

n. 

0 

200 

0 

300 

0 

200 

0 

200 

0 

300 

in. 

IV. 

V. 

VI. 

500 

700 

900 

1200 

500 

700 

ICX)0 

1200 

400 

700 

900 

1200 

500 

V^ 

1000 

1200 

500 

800 

1000 

1200 

93.  Balafong . 


South  Pacific. 
.  i    o  I    202    I      370    1      685    I      903    i    1200 


Javese  Scales,  as  observed  from  Instruments  and  Musicians. 


94.  Salendro,  observed 

95.  „        assumed 


0 

228 

484. 

728 

960 

1200 

0 

240 

480 

720 

960 

1200 

Javese  Pehg,  Chromatic  Scale,  from  which  tJie  others  are  selected. 


96.  The  seven  notes 


I. 

II. 

III. 

0 

137 

446 

IV. 

V. 

VI. 

575 

687 

820 

VII. 


1098 


VIII. 


1200 


The  Five-NoU  Scales  Selected, 


97.  Pelog 

98.  Dangsoe  {oe  as  in  shotf) 

99.  Bem 
100.  Barang    . 
loi.  Miring     . 
102.  Menjoera  (/o««- English  you) 


0 

— 

446 

575 

687 

— 

1098 

0 

1.37 

687 

820 

1098 

0 

137 

— 

575 

687 

— 

1098 

0 

137 

— 

575 

687 

820 

— 

0 

446 

575 

820 

1098 

0 

137 

446 

575 

— 

— 

1098 

1200  , 

1200 
1200 
1200 
1200 
1200 


Chinese  mdxed  Pentatonic  and  Heptatonic  Scales,  as  observed. 
*«*  Notes  marked  *  introdnoed  for  heptatonic  playing. 

103.  Flute  (Ti-tsu)  . 

104.  Oboe  (So-na)    . 

105.  Mouth-organ  (ShAng) 

106.  Grong-ehime  (Yun-lo) 

107.  Dulcimer  (Tang-ohin) 

108.  Tamboura  ^Sien-tsu) 

109.  Balloon  Omtar  (p*i-p'a) 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQIC 


0 

178 

*339 

448 

662 

888 

♦1103 

0 

145 

297 

440 

637 

8i3 

1014 

0 

210 

338 

498 

715 

908 

1040 

0 

169 

367 

586 

674 

775 

1062 

0 

169 

•274 

491 

661 

878 

♦996 

0 

189 

386 

702 

S^3 

0 

145 

35 » 

— 

647 

874 

— 

1 196 
I2I6 

1199 

I208 

Ii9« 
1200 

"95 


SISCT.  K. 


NON-HABMONIC   SCALES. 


5*9 


II.  Tablk  op  Non -Harmonic  Scales — continued. 


Pbntatonic  Scales — continued, 
Japanese,  chiefly  Pentatonic,  but  with  extra  notes  marked  * 

Koto  Tuning^  Popular  Scales, 
I.         II.      '      III. 


IV. 


V. 


no. 

Hiradioshi,  theoretical 

0 

204 

316      1 

702 

814  ; 

1200 

III. 

„          female  player 

0 

193 

357    : 

719 

801   , 

1 199 

112. 

„          musio-master 

0 

i8S 

337 

683 

790 

1200 

"3- 

Akebono    I.,  theore 

tical  . 

0 

200 

300    ! 

700 

900 

1200 

114. 

Akebono  II. 

0 

100 

500   ; 

700 

800 

1200 

115. 

Eumoi    I.               „ 

0 

100 

500   1 

700 

8oo 

1200 

116. 

Kumoi  II.               „ 

0 

100 

500   1 

700 

800 

1200 

117. 

Han-Kumoi            „ 

:    0 

200 

500 

700 

800    ; 

1200 

118. 

Kata-Kumoi           «, 

0 

200 

300 

700 

800    , 

1200 

119. 

Sakura                    „ 

0 

100      j         500 

700 

800  ; 

1200 

120. 

Iwato 

0 

ICO          500 

600 

1000 

1200 

121. 

Han-Iwato              „ 

0 

ICO    '      500 

700 

1000 

1200 

122. 

Kata-Iwato 

0 

100      1         500 

600 

1000 

1200 

123. 

Kumoi                    „ 

0 

100    ;     500 

700 

800 

1200 

Koto  Tuning,  Classical  Scales, 

124. 

Ichikotsu-Chio,  theoretical               .  1     0 

200 

500 

700 

900 

I200* 

"5. 

Hio-Dio 

0 

200 

500 

700 

900  j 

1200 

126. 

Sou-Dio                      ,, 

0 

200 

500 

700 

900  1 

1200 

127. 

Wausiki-Chio 

0 

200 

500 

70J 

9DO 

1200 

128. 

Sui-Dio 

0 

200    ■      500 

700 

lOOO 

1200 

129. 

Bausiki-Chio              „ 

0 

300         500 

700 

1000 

1200 

Heptatonii 

J  Scal4!s. 

I. 

I_n^j  m. 

IV. 

V. 

VI.          VII. 

1 

VIII. 

130. 

Classical  reosen,  theoretical    . 

0 

203       400 

♦600    700  1 

900 

*iioo 

1200 

131- 

,,       ritsasen         „ 

0 

200       300 

♦500  1  700 

2°° 

♦1000 

1200 

132. 

Popular    I.                    „          •        - 

0 

100       300 

500 

700  ' 

800 

1000 

1200 

'33. 

Popular  U. 

n 

. 

0 

100   1    . 

500 

500 

600  1 

800 

1000 

1200 

Japanese  Bvwa,  Classical  Instrument,  Tetrachords  observed  on  different  Strings. 

I.     I         II.         •         III.         t  IV.         I  V. 


I 


134.  Lowest  string  . 

135.  Second  lowest  . 

1 36.  Second  highest 

1 37.  Highest  string 

138.  Mean 

139.  Theoretically  assumed  as 


225    i 

33? 

416 

5'2 

223 

338 

429 

500 

195 

320 

407 

496 

212 

321 

414 

503 

214        1 

328 

416 

503 

200 

300 

400 

500 

III.  Annotations  to  the  Table. 


N08.  I  to  II  are  those  given  in  the  text, 
pp.  262-5,  ^^^  ^o*  8  was  merely  suggested 
by  Prof.  Helmholtz. 

N08.  12  to  24  are  from  App.  III.  to  Prof.  Land's 
paper,  Over  de  Toonladders  der  Arabische 
Musiek  (on  the  Arabic  musical  scales),  and 
contain  his  corrections  of  the  very  faulty 
MS.  of  Al  Farabi ;  the  numbers  are  also 
given  by  Eosegarteu,  p.  55.  After  24,  the 
numbers  suddenly  ^ease  in  the  MS. 

Nos.  25  to  31  are  the  old  theoretical  form  of 
the  Greek  scales  with  the  old  tetrachords, 
see  supr^,  p.  268  c,  d', 

Nos.  32  to  38  are  taken  from  supra,  p.  269  a. 

Nos.  39  to  50  are  from  Prof.  Land,  ibid.  p.  38, 
corrected  from  the  MS.  at  Leyden;  for 
No.  45  the  copyist  had  repeated  No.  44,  and 
Prof.  Land  has  supplied  the  numbers  by 
analog}'. 


No.  51  is  inferred  from  the  complete  set  of  ^ 
notes  nsed  on  the  Arabic  lute  at  dififerent 
times  as  shewn  in  No.  66, 

No.  52.  The  Highland  bagpipe  representing 
that  scale  has  been  inserted  immediately 
afterwards  to  show  its  practical  identity.  It 
was  played  to  Mr.  Hipkins  and  myself  by 
Mr.  C.  Keene,  the  artist. 

No.  53.  The  very  modem  survival  of  the 
same  scale  has  been  put  next.  It  is  de- 
scribed, supri,  p.  264  note  **.  In  practice 
each  note  might  be  sharpened  by  one  or 
more  quartertones. 

Nos.  54  to  65  are  the  twelve  scales  given, 
supra,  p.  2S4,  from  Prof.  Land,  but  the  four 
which  employ  8  notes  are  now  placed  last. 
No.  61,  Hhidjazi,  is,  in  fact,  more  harmonic 
than  the  nsual  equal  temperament.  If  we 
begin   on   the   note    vii.,    and   reckon    the 


Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


520 


ADDITIONS  BY  THE   TRANSLATOR. 


APP.  XX. 


intervals  from  it  through  an  octave,  after- 
wards  subtracting  996,  it  gives  the  scale 

0  204  384  498  702  882  1086  1200,  and  if 
384  882  1086  were  each  increased  by  2 
cents,  this  would  be  our  just  major  scale. 
The  difference  is  not  felt  even  in  chords,  as 

1  have  ascertained  by  actually  playing  them 
on  a  properly  tuned  concertina. 

No.  66  is  the  complete  collection  of  the  notes 
on  the  old  Arabic  lute,  as  used  at  different 
times,  reported  by  Prof.  Land.  Of  course 
the  Persian  and  ZalzaPs  notes  could  not  be 
used  together,  and  when  Zalzal's  355  and 
853  were  used,  both  294  and  408,  and  also 
botii  792  and  906  had  to  be  discontinued, 
producing  No.  51. 
No.  67  gives  the  complete  17  medieval  Arabic 
notes  as  determined  by  Prof.  Land,  with  the 

^  2  extra  ones  which  appear  in  the  second 
Octave.  Yilluteau  (op.  cit.  suprd,  p.  257, 
note  t,  ed.  1809,  folio,  vol.  i.)  declared,  as 
is  well  known,  that  the  most  generally  re- 
ceived Arabic  division  of  the  Octave  is  into 
thirds  of  a  Tone  (op.  cit.  p.  613).  Prof. 
Land  has  demonstrated  (Gamme  Arabe, 
p.  62)  that  this  is  not  the  case.  Villoteau, 
ftn  excellent  musician,  sent  to  Egypt  by 
the  French  Government  to  study  the  native 
music,  had  every  facility  given  to  him,  and 
had  native  musicians  at  his  beck  and  calL 
How  did  he  arrive  at  this  opinion  ?  After 
an  attentive  study  of  his  book  I  consider 
the  following  hypothesis  probable.  The 
greater  number  of  theorists  gave  17  notes 
to  the  Octave.  This  was  the  medieval 
Arabic  scale,  No.  67.  Villoteau  was  not 
used  to  just  intervals,  and  he  was  a  very 

^  poor  arithmetician  (see  the  remarkable  note 
op.  cit.  p.  668).  He  was  used  to  the  *  musi- 
cians' cycle '  of  55  degrees  (supr&,  p.  436d, 
viii.),  in  which  tiie  Tone  contained  9,  the 
major  Semitone  5,  and  the  minor  4  degrees 
(op.  cit.  pp.  667,  678).  When  he  heard  the 
scale  of  liaat  played  (see  p.  284  and  No.  57, 
for  the  medieval  form  o  204  384  498  702 
882  996  1200  cents),  which  was  the  principal 
Egyptian  scale,  he  tried  to  sing  it  as  ^1  £ 
CK  ,  &c.,  in  his  55  degrees,  but  was  imme- 
diately told  that  his  CZ  ~392  cents  was 
too  sharp.  This  would  hardly  have  been 
the  case  if  the  true  medieval  384  had  been 
played  to  him.  He  next  tried  A  B  C  =  305 
cents,  but  then  C  was  too  flat.  Now  the 
interval  Cix>  Ct  - 87  cents  was  his  minor 
Semitone  of  4  degrees.  Hence  he  concluded 
that  3  degrees  =65  cents  in  his  tempera- 

^  ment  would  be  right,  and  that  satisfied  the 
natives  (op.  cit.  p.  679).  This,  however,  was 
one  third  of  his  Tone.  But  he  found  also 
17  tabaqat  or  transpositions  of  each  scale, 
proceeding  by  Fourths,  called  by  him  per- 
fect, but  as  he  really  considered  1 7  of  these 
Fourths  to  make  up  7  Octaves,  he  arrived  at 
a  cycle  of  17  degrees,  each  having  71  cents 
(ex.  70-588233)  or  being  almost  precisely  a 
small  Semitone  24  :  25  (  =  ex.  70*673  cents). 
To  the  nearest  cent,  using  his  symbols, 
where  x  means  increased  by  one  third,  and 
t  increased  by  two  thirds  of  a  Tone,  and 
i,  b  mean  diminished  by  the  same  amount), 
the  notes  of  this  cycle  in  cents  were,  1^0, 

2  Ax  «Sb  71,  3  A^  =jBU  141,  4  B  212, 
5  C  282,  6  Cx  =i)b  353.  7  Ct^^Dl  424, 
8  D  494,  gDx^Eb  565.  10  Dn  "El  635, 
IX  E  706,  12  F  776,  13  Fx  «Gb  847,  14 


FZ  -Gl  918,  15  G988,  16  Gx  =^b  io5». 
17  Ot  ''Al  1 129,  i'  A  1200.  Then  b« 
writes  the  Bast  as  he  heard  it,  as  ii  B  C  x 
D  E  Fx  G  Ay  which  therefore  gave  the 
cents  o  212  353  494  706  847  988  1200.  Now 
it  is  difficult  to  conceive  that  he  could  have 
heard  the  medieval  Bast  in  this  way,  even 
though  the  intervals  were  determined  purely 
by  estimation  of  ear,  apparently  his  only 
method  of  estimation.  But  the  probability 
is  that  medieval  Bast^  like  the  other  me- 
dieval scales,  had  become  a  thing  of  the 
past,  and  that  what  Villoteau  heard  in 
Egypt  in  1800  was  what  Eli  Smith  in  1849 
tells  us  Meshaqah  (supr&,  p.  2646)  laid  down 
at  Damascus,  namely  No.  53,  that  is,  the 
normal  scale  o  200  350  500  700  850  1000 
1200  cents,  a  survived  of  Zalzal's  with  the 
neutral  Third  and  Sixth,  and  this  is  very 
accurately  represented  by  the  above  scale  of 
Kast,  as  Villoteau  notes  it.  At  the  very 
outset  Villoteau  says  that  some  divide  the 
Octave  into  Tones,  Semitones,  and  Quarter- 
tones  (op.  cit.  p.  6x3).  This  shews  that  the 
24  division  was  even  acknowledged.  But 
ViUotean  was  perfectly  ignorant  of  equal 
temperament,  and  hence  paid  no  attention 
to  this.  On  the  other  hand  he  found  the  17 
divisions  in  the  theorists,  and  made  them 
equal,  because  he  thus  seemed  to  reconcile 
theory  and  practice.  But  he  only  obtained 
an  outline  thus,  as  is  evidently  shewn  by 
his  speaking  (op.  oit.  p.  612)  of  *  les  divi- 
sions et  subdivisions  des  tons  de  la  mnsique 
arabe  en  intervalles  si  petits  et  si  pen  na- 
turels,  que  I'ouie  ne  peut  jamais  les  saisir 
avec  une  pr^ision  exacte,  ni  la  voix  les 
entonner  avec  une  parfaite  jnstesse.'  It 
was  evident  there  were  many  other  Tones 
(the  Quartertones)  not  in  his  list  of  17. 
Lideed  he  says  (op.  oit.  p.  673) :  *  Us  savent 
anssi  qu'il  y  a  d'autres  degr^s  interm6diaires 
aux  pr6c6dents,  et  ils  en  font  usage  mtoe 
assez  fr^quemment,  mais  ils  ne  sauraient 
dire  au  juste  quelle  est  la  nature  et  P^tendne 
de  rintervalle  qui  s^pare  ces  degr^  les 
uns  des  autres.'  These  were  possibly  the 
Quartertones  (very  uncertainly  produced)  by 
which  we  learn  from  Eli  Smith  that  the 
Arabs,  like  the  Lidians,  continually  varied 
their  scale.  If  the  17  thirds  of  tones  of 
Villoteau,  just  given  in  his  notation,  be 
read  as  i  C,  2  Db,  3  ^bb.  4  A  5  ^b,  6  fb, 
y  E,  S  Fy  9  Gb,  10  ^bb,  11  O,  12  Ab, 
13  Bbb,  14  A,  15  £b,  16  Cb,  17  Dbb,  as  in 
No.  67,  his  12  scales  will  be  found  to  cor- 
respond precisely  in  names  of  the  notes 
with  those  given  by  Prof.  Land  (snpri, 
p.  284),  but  the  whole  of  the  intervals, 
which  were  originally  of  90  or  24  cents,  are 
now  equalised  as  71  cents  by  this  confused 
temperament  of  Villotean*s  in  which  medi- 
eval Arabic  music  seems  to  have  been  in- 
tended, but  the  modem  form  was  reaDy 
misrepresented.  To  shut  up  24  Quarter- 
tones  into  17  thirds  of  Tones,  at  least  two 
must  be  given  to  one  note  on  seven  occa- 
sions. Thus  (in  cents)  Villoteau*s  7 1  was 
50  or  100,  his  2S2  was  250  or  300,  his  424 
was  400  or  450,  his  565  was  550  or  600, 
his  776  was  750  or  800,  his  918  was  900  or 
950,  and  his  11 29  was  1 100  or  1150.  This 
would  fully  account  for  the  indistinctness 
complained  of. 
No.  68  is  a  complete  Arabic  medieval  scale. 
Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


SECT.  K. 


NON-HARMONIC  SCALES. 


521 


with  additional  intervals,  612,  816,  mo, 
1200+24,  1200+  114,  played  on  a  tambour. 
These  very  long-neoked  guitars  iJlow  of 
minute  subdivision  of  the  string. 

Kos.  69  to  71  are  the  various  notes  produced 
by  the  Bab&b,  according  to  the  three  methods 
of  tuning  the  second  string  as  316,  408,  or 
590  cents,  the  intervals  between  pairs  of 
notes  on  both  strings  being  identical.  In 
No.  69  the  note  520  cents  is  played  on  the 
second  string,  but  is  here  inserted  in  order 
of  pitch. 

Ko.  72  gives  the  most  extraordinary  and  most 
limited  scale  known,  produced  by  using  only 
the  open  string  and  39,  38,  37,  36,  and  35 
fortieths  of  it ;  the  open  second  string  being 
tuned  in  unison  with  the  sharpest  note  of 
the  first  string.  It  is  valuable  as  showing 
a  primitive  method  of  obtaining  scales  and 
a  division  of  one>eighth  of  the  keyboard 
into  5  equal  parts. 

Kos.  73  and  74  are  an  attempt  to  represent 
the  Indian  Chromatic  Scale  fiom  indications 
in  Bajah  Sourindro  Mohun  Tagore's  Musical 
Scales  of  the  Hindus^  Calcutta,  1884,  and 
the  Annvaire  du  Conservatoire  de  Bruxelles^ 
1878,  pp.  16 1 -1 69,  the  latter  having  been 
drawn  up  by  Mons.  V.  Mahillon  from  in- 
formation furnished  by  the  Bajah.  As 
regards  the  7  flxed  notes  {prahrita)  of  the  C 
scale  (sharja  gr(Una),  C,  D,  JS7,  F,  G,  A  (a 
comma  sharper  than  our  Ay)^  £,  there 
seems  to  be  no  doubt  of  the  theoretical 
values.  As  to  the  12  changing  notes 
{vikrita)t  the  values  given  can  be  con- 
sidered only  as  approximative.  The  divi- 
sion of  the  intervals  of  a  major  Tone  of  204 
cents  into  4  degrees  (s'rtUis) ;  of  a  minor 
Tone  of  182  cents  into  3  degrees;  and  of 
a  Semitone  of  112  cents  into  2  degrees,  as 
indicated  by  the  superscribed  numbers,  is 
also  certain.  But  whether  the  4  parts  of  a 
whole  Tone  were  equal  and  each  51  cents, 
and  the  three  parts  of  a  minor  Tone  were 
also  equal  and  each  equal  to  60}  cents,  and 
the  two  parts  of  a  Semitone  were  also  equal 
and  each  therefore  56  cents,  is  quite  un- 
certain. This,  however,  was  assumed  to  be 
the  case  in  calculating  No.  73,  and  the 
results  are  probably  not  much  out.  Nor  is 
it  likely  that  the  alterations  by  degrees 
(produced  on  increasing  the  tension  of  the 
string  by  pressing  behind  high  frets,  or 
deflecting  the  string  along  low  frets,  or  by 
arranging  the  movable  frets)  were  even 
approximately  constant.  In  No.  74  we 
have  the  ipodern  Bengali  division  of  the 
finger-board  referred  to  in  the  above  books. 
It  seems  that  the  string  is  first  divided  into 
half  and  a  quarter,  giving  the  Octave  and 
Ponrth  (theoretically).  Then  the  distance 
from  the  First  to  the  Fourth  on  the  finger- 
board is  divided  into  9  equal  parts,  and  that 
from  the  Fourth  to  the  Octave  into  13  equal 
parts,  and  each  distance  represents  the 
interval  of  a  degree  (s'ruti).  From  these 
data  the  values  of  No.  74  have  been  calcu- 
lated. It  will  be  seen  by  subtraction  that 
the  first  9  degrees  thus  found  vary  from  49 
to  63  cents,  and  the  last  13  from  45  to  64 
cents.  The  cents  found,  however,  from  the 
inverse  ratio  of  the  lengths  will  differ 
slightly  from  those  used  practically.  The 
S  and  b  (tihra  and  komala)  are  used  in  this 
scale  for  deviations  of  two  degrees  when  a 


major  tone  is  divided,  and  for  deviations  of 
one  degree  when  a  minor  Tone  or  Semitone 
is  divided.  This  is  done  by  the  Bajah  in 
his  translations  into  ordinary  notation.  In 
addition  I  have  taken  the  liberty  to  use  bb 
to  represent  only  one  degree  flatter  than  the 
single  b,  and  wish  it  to  be  read  *  very  flat ' 
(ati-komala) ;  similarly  8  S  is  one  degree 
sharper  than  %  ,  and  should  be  read  *  very 
sharp*  {ati-tibra).  The  Bajah  not  having 
distinguished  the  very  flat  and  very  sharp 
notes  from  the  simply  flat  and  sharp  ones 
in  his  304  scales,  I  have  avoided  citing  them 
at  length.  Similarly  I  have  not  been  able 
satisfactorily  to  find  the  cents  for  the  F 
scale  or  mad*hyama  grdma  (usually  repre- 
sented as  our  just  major  scale  in  which 
A  should  be  one  comma  instead  of  one 
degree  flatter  than  in  the  O  scale,  as  it  ^ 
would  appear  to  be),  or  for  the  E  scale  or 
gand*hdra  grdma  (in  which  D  and  A  appear 
to  be  one  degree  flatter,  and  B  one  degree 
sharper  than  in  the  normal  C  scale  or 
sharja  grdma),  and  hence  I  have  not  given 
them  in  the  table.  But,  using  numbers 
before  the  notes  for  degrees,  they  may 
possibly  be  for  the  F  scale,  iC,  5l>.  8£, 
loF,  14G,  17 A,  21B,  which  would  use 
degree  17,  and  the  corresponding  note  may 
be  called  grave  A,  and  written  A\  For  the 
E  scale  we  may  possibly  have  iC,  4i),  SE, 
loF,  14G,  17 A,  22B,  where  4D  is  now 
utilised,  and  becomes  grave  D\  But  these 
A\  D\  are  not  our  il„  Dp  and  hence  the 
scales  are  different  from  ours.  This  is, 
however,  pure  conjecture. 

No.  75,  for  the  old  national  Indian  instru- 
ment, a  Vina  from  Madras,  in  the  South  ^ 
Kensington  Museum,  gives  the  value  of 
24  notes  by  measuring  vibrating  lengths  of 
string  from  fret  to  bridge,  and  is,  of  course, 
very  uncertain.  It  will  be  found,  however, 
that  the  notes  agree  with  No.  74  better  than 
with  No.  73,  for  the  scale  C  Db  D  Eb  E 
F  Ft  O  Ab  A  Bb  B  c.  The  other  degrees 
could  be  easily  produced  by  pressing  the 
string  behind  the  frets,  which  were  about 
one  inch  in  height. 

Nos.  76  to  80  are  five  observations  of  scales 
played  by  Bajah  lUun  P&l  Singh,  and  ob- 
served with  forks.  These  were  set  by  alter- 
ing the  movable  frets  of  a  sit4r.  The  first 
and  fourth  are  placed  together  in  the  table, 
as  they  are  believed  to  have  been  meant  for 
the  same  scale,  and  differed  only  because 
they  were  set  on  different  days.  They  seem 
to  be  meant  for  Bajah  Sourindro  Mohun  ^ 
Tagore's  first  scale.  The  second  setting 
seems  meant  for  his  13th,  the  third  for  his 
29th,  and  the  fifth  for  his  9th. 

No.  81.  A  wood  harmonicon  in  the  South  Ken- 
sington Museum,  stated  to  have  come  from 
Patna,  but  probably  arrived  from  some  hill 
tribes.  Its  scale  resembles  one  which  I 
deduced  by  measurements  of  strings  from 
a  Tar  of  Cashmere,  which  was  o  175  354 
512  720  896  1062  1237,  but  I  thought  this 
scale  too  uncertain  to  put  in  the  table. 

No.  82.  A  wood  harmonicon  sent  direct  from 
Singapore  to  Mr.  A.  J.  Hipkins,  taking  the 
central  Octave. 

Nos.  83  to  87.  Wood  harmonicons  in  South 
Kensington  Museum,  of  which  the  last  be- 
longed to  General  Pitt-Bivers. 

Nos.  88  to  92  are  inserted  because  theyare  the 
Digitized  by  V^jDOQlC 


522 


ADDITIONS  BY  THE  TRANSLATOR. 


APP.  XX, 


examples  of  pentatonio  soales  asnally  giTen. 
They  are  at  any  rate  now  used  for  penta- 
tonic  Scotch  masic.  See  supr^,  p.  259^2.  They 
are  not,  however,  by  any  means  usual  forms. 

No.  93.  A  balafong  from  the  South  Pacific  be- 
longing to  General  Pitt-Bivers,  seems  to  be 
intended  for  No.  90,  but  370,  685  cents  for 
400  and  700  cents  are  both  rather  flat. 

Nob.  94  to  102.  Javese  instruments  examined 
at  the  Aquarium,  London,  in  1882,  and  pitch 
of  notes  determined  by  forks.  In  Nos.  96 
to  100  the  order  of  these  notes  settled  by 
Mr.  W.  Stephen  Mitchell,  and  the  order 
conflrmed  by  information  received  through 
Prof.  Land  of  Leyden.  Nos.  loi  and  102  were 
inferred  from  information  of  missionaries 
obtained  by  Prof.  Land.  The  assumption 
in  No.  95  of  a  division  of  the  Octave  into 
^  five  equal  parts  was  confirmed  by  other 
measurements  communicated  by  Prof.  Land. 

Nos.  103  to  109.  These  seven  scales  were  taken 
from  the  playing  of  Chinese  musicians  at 
the  InternationaJ  Health  Exhibition,  1885, 
during  four  private  interviews.  No.  106  had 
two  additional  tones  of  497  and  797  cents 
above  the  lowest  note ;  these  were  omitted 
in  playing  by  the  musician.  There  was  a 
second  yiin-lo  at  the  South  Kensington  Mu- 
seum, also  with  10  tones,  which  gave  o  52 
240  266  418  437  586  589  712  738,  but  these 
form  no  scale.  They  may  be  a  fund  out  of 
which  scales  are  constructed.  The  four  fol- 
lowing may  be  among  such: — To  a  sharp 
Fifth,  o  240  418  589  712;  to  a  flat  Fifth, 
o  188  366  534  686;  to  a  flat  Fifth  again, 
o  214  385  537  686;  to  a  fourth,  o  197  349 
498.  The  last  is  a  Meshaqah  tetrachord, 
il  see  No.  53.  A  chime  of  four  small  bells,  be- 
longing to  Mr.  Hermann  Smith,  gave  o  312 
480724. 

Nos.  no  to  139.  Japanese  scales.  Accepting 
the  statement  of  native  musicians  that  the 
intervals  are  those  of  equal  temperament, 
or  at  least  so  near  them  that  Japanese  ears 
do  not  perceive  tl^ie  difference,  then  the 
theory  gives  Nos.  no  and  Nos.  113  to  128 
for  *  popular*  koto  tunings.  There  are  13 
strings  to  the  koto,  but  only  5  give  the  scale, 


the  rest  being  Octaves  or  onisons.  Nos.  1 1 1 
and  112,  heard  at  the  *  Japanese  Village' 
Knightsbridge,  shew  how  practice  sometimea 
overrides  theory.  Several  of  the  scales  seem 
to  be  identical,  but  they  are  at  different 
pitches,  and  henoe  no  more  identical  than 
our  major  scales  for  different  keys. 

Nos.  124  to  129  are  '  elassical '  koto  tamngs  of 
which  124  126  128  are  classed  as  rio$en  or 
analogous  to  our  major  scales,  and  125  127 
129  as  ritsusen  or  analogous  to  oar  minor 
scales.  This  must  have  been  effected  by 
sharpening  some  of  the  notes  by  pressure 
on  the  string  behind  the  bridges  which  limit 
their  vibrating  length. 

Nos.  130  and  131  are  both  *  classical '  hepta- 
tonic  scales,  owing  to  the  introdaotion  of 
the  notes  marked  *. 

Nos.  132  and  133  are  both  *  popular '  hepta- 
tonic  scales,  in  which  however  the  intro- 
duced notes  are  not  pointed  out  in  Mr. 
Isawa's  Report  on  Japanese  music  at  the 
Health  Exhibition  of  1885,  Educational 
Division,  from  which  the  scales  No.  1 10  and 
Nos.  113  to  133  have  been  taken. 

Nos.  134  to  139  result  from  an  examination  of 
the  Biwa,  a  classical  instrument,  closely  re- 
sembling the  Arabic  lute,  fretted  only  as  far 
as  the  Fourth.  The  strings  are  tuned  to 
one  another  in  six  different  ways,  and  hence 
produce  a  great  variety  of  notes.  By  touch- 
ing the  strings  on  the  frets  (taking  care  not 
to  press  behind  them)  and  determining  the 
pitch  of  the  notes  sounded,  Mr.Hipkins  and 
I  found  that  the  tetrachord  produced  dif- 
fered according  to  the  string  employed,  as 
shewn  in  Nos.  134  to  137.  The  mean  of  the 
intervals  thus  determined  is  given  in  No.  138, 
and  accepting  equal  temperament,  as  in  the 
whole  of  Mr.  Isawa's  report,  the  correspond- 
ing divisions  are  given  in  No.  139.  These 
divisions  were  in  so  far  assumed  to  be  cor- 
rect that  the  three  Semitones,  though  ma- 
terially differing  (having  the  mean  vadnes  of 
114,  89,  and  86  cents),  are,  in  Mr.  Isawa's 
account  of  the  notes,  considered  as  alike, 
and  exactly  half  of  the  value  of  first  Tone, 
which  had  a  mean  value  of  214  cents. 


IV.  How  THESE  Divisions  of  the  Octave  may  have  abiben. 

It  is  impossible  to  trace  such  scales  to  their  germs.  Singing  and  playing  on 
pipes  were  probably  the  first  music.  Striking  of  bone,  wooden,  and  metal  bars  was 
^  probably  also  a  very  early  form,  and  as  their  notes  are  tolerably  persistent  they  are 
valuable  for  determining  scales  (see  Nos.  81  to  87  and  93  to  102).  But  scales  them- 
selves are  a  great  development,  and  two  or  three  notes,  varied  rhythmically,  pro- 
bably long  preceded  them.  We  must  be  content  to  commence  with  strings,  certunly 
a  very  late  form  of  musical  instrument,  on  which,  however,  the  chief  work  of  the 
older  theorists  was  expended.  After  the  examples  in  the  latter  part  of  the  Table 
we  have  no  right  to  assume  an  accurate  musical  *  ear,*  or  appreciation  of  just 
intervals.  Even  in  Europe  it  requires  much  practice  for  the  majority  to  sing 
accurately  in  tune  or  to  appreciate  small  errors.  (See  suprii-,  p.  1476?.)  We  now 
know  that  on  any  stringed  instrujnent  such  as  the  violoncello  or  guitar,  where,  to 
prevent  jarring,  the  string  has  to  rise  further  and  further  from  the  finger  board 
as  the  finger  proceeds  from  the  nut  towards  the  bridge,  the  pressure  of  the  finger 
in  *  stopping  '  the  string  either  on  the  board  or  fret  increases  the  tension  of  the 
string  and  hence  makes  the  note  sharper  than  it  would  be  if  the  string  could  be 
stopped  at  its  natural  height,  though  even  tlien,  as  we  have  seen  (p.  442a),  the 
results  are  not  absolutely  trustworthy.     The  law  that  the  number  of  vibrations  is 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


SECT.  K. 


NON-HARMONIC  SCALES. 


523 


inyersely  proportional  to  the  length  of  the  string  holds  hut  very  roughly  for  such 
instruments.  Thus  stopping  a  violoncello  string  exactly  at  its  middle  gives  a  note 
sharper  than  the  Octave,  hence  the  finger  has  to  he  placed  sensihly  nearer  the  nut. 
Moreover,  the  amount  of  error  depends  on  the  nature  of  the  string.  The  examples 
Nob.  134  to  137  in  the  tahle  are  very  instructive.  The  determination  with  which 
those  differences  are  overlooked  (see  Annotations  to  these  Nos.)  is  equally 
instructive.  It  is  evident  that  Euclid  in  his  Canon  for  the  Pythagorean  notes  No. 
32,  and  Ahdulqadir  in  giving  his  rule  for  obtaining  the  17  notes  of  his  Ecale  No. 
67,  considered  the  division  to  be  perfect,  and  Prof.  Land  in  calculating  the  value 
of  the  notes  had,  of  course,  to  assume  that  it  was  so.  The  old  intervals  were, 
therefore,  not  so  accurately  tuned  as  was  supposed,  and  hence  when  we  take  them 
to  be  accurately  tuned  we  are  ourselves  inaccurate. 


The  fact  was  strongly  impressed  on  me  in 
making  an  instrument  on  which  I  could  play 
any  scale  expressed  in  cents.  I  had  a  Dichord, 
that  is,  a  double  monoohord,  constructed  with 
wires  1,200  millimetres  long,  diameter  *3  mm., 
height  of  nut  7  mm.,  of  bridge  24mm.,  from 
soimd  board.  Then  having  a  number  of  laths 
5  mm.  thick,  I  used  them  as  moveable  finger- 
boards, and  marked  on  one  the  place  of  the 
notes  of  the  just  scale  as  determined  by  the 
theory  of  inverse  ratios.  Trying,  by  my  Har- 
monical,  I  found  every  place  much  too  sharp, 
and  it  was  only  by  marking  the  places  which 
gave  unisons  that  I  was  able  to  correct  the 
error.  If  any  one  constructs  such  an  instru- 
ment, I  recommend  his  setting  off  the  place 
where  he  should  stop  for  each  semitone  for 
two  octaves,  by  a  well -tuned  pianoforte,  and 
then  dividing  each  distance  representing  a 
semitone  into  10  parts.  Each  of  these  parts 
will  represent  10  cents  with  quite  sufficient  ac- 
curacy, and  can  be  subdivided  by  the  eye.  Thus 
a  geometrical  scale  can  be  constructed  by 
means  of  which  the  places  to  touch  the  string 
can  be  marked  oft  on  a  new  lath,  and  the  scale 
played.  This  geometrical  scale  was  placed 
under  one  string  and  the  finger-board  to  be 
played  from  under  the  other.  I  found  it  best 
for  accuracy  to  stop  the  string  with  the  side 
of  my  thumb-nail.  All  the  principal  scales 
above  given  were  thus  realised. 

Now  assuming  the  usual  law  of  division, 
suppose  a  string  divided  in  half,  giving  the 
Octave,  and  each  half  subdivided  in  half,  giving 
the  Fourth  and  double  Octave.  This  division 
being  the  simplest  possible  would  naturally 
give  a  preponderance  to  the  Fourth,  whence 
would  arise  the  tetrachords,  the  foundation  of 
Greek,  and  hence  of  European,  and  of  Persian 
and  Arabic  music.  The  Fourth  is  also  recog- 
nised in  India;  but  in  pentatonic  regions, 
especially  in  Java,  where  the  string  is  not  in 
use  (the  rab4b  they  use  is  Arabic  in  name  and 
origm,  Nos.  69  to  71),  the  Fourth  is  not  cor- 
rect. It  is  clearly,  therefore,  not  a  primitive 
interval,  and  the  quartering  of  the  string  may 
really  have  much  to  do  with  its  adoption. 

The  interval  was,  however,  too  wide,  and  it 
was  necessary  to  subdivide  it.  The  most  ob- 
vious plan  was  again  to  quarter  it.  Thus,  the 
additional  distances  of  ^,  ^  »  i,  ^  of  the  string 
from  the  nut  would  be  obtained,  giving  the 
vibrating  lengths  ^|,  {,  ^,  the  ratio.  The  first 
gives  the  diatonic  Semitone  of  1 1 2  cents,  and 
its  defect  from  the  Fourth,  498  cents,  the  major 
Third  of  386  cents,  see  No.  i.  This  ^  of  the 
string  «  1 12  cents  is  conspicuous  in  Nos.  i  to 
4,  some  of  the  oldest  forms.  The  J,  «  23 1  cents, 
we  find  in  No.  72.    The  Jf  =  360  cents  did  not 


come  into  use,  but  it  is  practically  Zalzal's 
355  cents.  No.  51. 

Continuing  this  simplest  of  all  subdivisions  ^ 
by  two,  we  have  half  of  ^^^  »  ^^  of  the  string 
from  the  nut,  giving  the  vibrating  length  §|  of 
the  string  =55  cents.  Hence  we  obtain  the 
enharmonic  division  No.  11.  At  the  same 
time  my  observations  on  p.  265,  note  *,  hold 
good,  for  the  errors  in  coming  so  near  the  nut 
as  ^  of  the  string,  would  be  too  great  to  ob- 
tain anything  like  accurate  results  by  measure- 
ment. On  my  dichord  I  found  it  impossible 
to  take  less  than  a  semitone  of  100  cents  VTith 
any  degree  of  certainty.  It  is  interesting  to 
observe  that  this  JJ  of  the  string  gives  very 
nearly  50  cents  or  the  Quartertone,  and  still 
more  nearly  54^,  the  22nd  part  of  an  Octave, 
corresponding  to  the  Indian  degree  (Nos.  73 
and  74  Annotation),  and  is  really  the  com- 
mencement of  the  variation  of  notes  by  about 
a  Quartertone. 

Then  the  divisions  attempted  in  the  South-  % 
em  Tambour  No.  72,  and  also  in  forming 
the  Persian  middle-finger  note,  303  cents  (see 
No.  66),  by  taking  a  place  half-way  between  that 
for  294  cents  and  408  cents,  and  again  for  Zal- 
zars  middle-finger  note  of  355  cents,  by  taking 
a  place  half-way  between  303  and  408  cents^ 
and  finally  the  modem  Bengali  division  of  the 
distance  occupied  by  a  Fourth  on  the  finger- 
board into  9  parts  (see  No.  74)  and  that  for 
the  following  Fifth  into  13  parts,  suggest  that 
the  attempts  were  made  to  divide  the  ^  of  the 
string  from  the  nut  to  the  Fourth,  by  other 
simple  numbers  beside  2.  The  division  into 
three  parts  would  be  more  difficult,  but  might 
be  done  very  fairly  by  guess.  Now  the  dis- 
tances of  the  stopping-place  from  the  nut  ■^, 
IS  =  a  o'  *^®  string  or  the  vibrating  lengths  {^ 
and  I  of  the  string  corresponding  to  151  cents  or 
and  316  cents,  the  first,  the  Threequartertone,  ' 
which  is  the  real  parent  of  all  the  neutral 
intervals  to  be  considered  presently,  and  the 
second  the  minor  Third.  Both  occur  in 
No.  10,  and  the  minor  Third  occurs  also  in 
Nos.  3  and  6. 

The  division  of  the  whole  string  into  thirds 
could  hardly  have  taken  place,  but  ^  of  the 
string  from  nut  gives  the  vibrating  length  ^  of 
the  string  «  702  cents,  the  Fifth,  which,  as 
exceeding  the  Fourth  of  498  cents,  would  not 
be  regarded  till  the  tetrachord  had  been  ex- 
tended to  the  Octave.  The  defect  of  a  Fourth 
from  a  Fifth  gave  the  major  Tone,  one  of  the 
most  important  intervals,  also  obtained  di- 
rectly by  taking  ^  of  i,  or  ^  of  the  string  from 
the  nut,  giving  {  of  the  strmg  as  the  vibrating 
length  =  204  cents.  This  interval  finally  ab- 
sorbed  all  the   others,    except    the    Fourth, 


Digitized  by  VjOOQlC 


524 


ADDITIONS  BY  THE  TRANSLATOB. 


APP.  XX. 


espeoially  after  the  observation  that  it  was 
also  the  defect  of  two  Fourths  from  an  Octave. 
Its  direct  use  is  apparent  in  No.  19,  where  204 
cents  corresponds  to  i  string  from  nut,  and 
408  cents  to  j^  of  the  vibrating  length  of  the 
204  cents  from  the  stop  for  204  cents.  Thus 
if  the  nut  be  called  A^  and  the  stopping  places 
for  204,  408,  and  498  be  B^  C,  D,  and  the 
bridge  be  Z,  we  shall  have  AD'^\  AZ^  whence 
i>Z  =  |  AZ\  also  AB  =  l  AZ,  whence  BZ^ 
%  AZ\  also  BC  =  |  BZ,  whence  CZ  =  g  BZ  = 
jj^  AZ,  If  we  suppose  the  whole  length  AZ 
divided  into  324  parts,  then  AB  =  36,  BC  «  32, 
Ci>»  13.  But  the  whole  division  is  obtained 
by  taking  halves  and  thirds.  Ko.  5  is  the 
reverse  of  No.  19.  Let  the  stopping-places 
for  90  and  294  be  E  and  F,  Then  DZ^\  AZ, 
FZ-I  DZ  =  DZ-\-\  DZ,  so  that  F  is  found 

f  Jrom  DZ  by  adding  ^  DZ^  which  is  obtained 
by  continual  halving.  Again  EZ  =  \  FZ  = 
FZ  +  i  FZ,  so  that  E  is  found  from  FZ  by 
adding  ^  FZ,  which  is  again  obtained  by  con- 
tinual halving.  This  made  it  easier  to  produce 
No.  5  than  No.  19.  The  complicated  value  of 
AE^^AZ  would  be  thus  altogether  avoided. 
Nevertneless  it  is  most  probable  that  Nos.  5 
and  19  were  both  obtained  simply  *  by' ear,' 
and  that  they  were  never  exactly  *  in  tune.' 

The  next  division  of  the  Fourth  to  be  ex- 
pected is  by  5,  as  in  No.  72,  and  ^is  is  there- 
fore advanced  by  Prof.  Land  as  a  probable 
means  of  obtaining  the  Arabic  tetrachord. 
This  gives  the  lengths  of  the  string  from  the 
»«!*»  551  TO  *  A»  A»  A  ^^  i»  55  -  }» aJ^d  the  vibrating 
lengths  fi,  A,  JJ,  ±,  5  of  the  string  giving  inter- 
vals of  59,  182,  281,  386,  489  cents.    Of  these 

^  89  is  a  fair  representative  of  90  in  No.   5, 


which  has  just  been  otherwise  obtained.  The 
minor  Tone,  182  cents,  occurs  direct  in  Nos.  6 
and  7,  and  also  possibly  in  No.  2»  where  it 
would  be  simpler  to  obtain  it  from  the  open 
string  as  ^  its  length  than  as  {{  of  the  vibratiiig 
lenjg^  of  112  cents,  that  is,  as  |}  of  ^  of  the 
string.  The  281  cents  appears  not  to  have 
been  used,  but  it  approximates  to  294  cents, 
which  may  have  been  introduced  from  Gxeeoe 
in  place  of  it.  This  is,  however,  mere  con- 
jecture. The  386,  or  major  Third,  is  in  No.  7 
only  (for  No.  8  is  not  ancient),  and  there  is 
very  little  probability  that  it  was  tuned  direct. 
It  might  have  been  got  as  182  +  204  or  as 
498  —  112.  Both  the  182  and  386  cents  wexe 
certainly  lost  at  an  early  time  in  204  and  408 
cents,  so  that  it  is  difficult  to  suppose  that 
386  at  least  was  ever  obtained  directly.  It 
was  indirectly  produced  in  Nos.  i  and  2, 
where,  judging  from  Japanese  habits,  the 
tuner  tried  to  get  the  Semitone  by  '  feeling/ 
and  left  the  major  Third  to  arise  as  the  defect 
of  the  Semitone  from  the  Fourth. 

The  division  into  7  parts  belongs  to  a  mach 
more  advanced  stage,  and  never  seems  to  have 
come  into  use.  But  we  may  understand  No.  9 
thus.  Stopping  at  \  the  string,  we  obtain  a 
vibrating  length  of  J  ^  267  cents.  Then  taking 
I  of  ^,  or  ^  the  length  for  the  stopping  place, 
we  obtain  a  vibrating  length  of  ^  string « 85 
cents,  and  thus  find  both  intervals  in  No.  9. 
Of  course  when  the  ball  had  been  set  tolling, 
and  there  was  no  harmony  to  check  the 
fancies  of  dividers  or  musicians,  such  forms  as 
Nos.  12  to  24  could  be  produced.  But  the 
ancient  Nos.  I  to  7  and  9  to  1 1  (No.  8  was  not 
ancient)  are  sufficient  to  have  traced. 


V.  Besults  op  the  Inquiry. 

The  chief  points  of  interest  which  the  exhibition  of  these  scales  afifords  appear 
to  be  the  following : 

I.  The  predominance  of  the  Fourth,  and  mere  evolution  of  the  Fifth,  in  Greece, 
Arabia,  India,  and  Japan. 


These  may  be  only  different  forms  of  some 
original  system.  The  Chinese  may  have 
imported  the  principle,  but  on  this  the  ex- 
treme uncertainty  pervading  all  exhibitions  of 
Chinese  scales  hitherto  made  (including  Van 
Aalst's  treatise  on  Chinese  Music,  1884)  renders 
it  difficult  to  judge.  The  Fourths  actually 
heard  are  uncertain,  see  Nos.  103  to  109.  But 
they  seem  at  home  in  Japan,  where  they  are 
m  used  in  tuning,  but  may  have  been  imported, 
and  they  are  occasionally  absent.  In  the  im- 
portant Javese  scales,  Nos.  94  to  102,  they  are 
never  in  tune.  Even  in  Arabia  and  India  they 
are  apt  to  be  altered.    The  specimens  of  ruder 


music.  Nob.  81  to  87,  are  not  favourable  to  the 
Fourth.  The  Fifth  never  had  the  same  pre- 
dominance. It  is  constantly  too  sharp  or  too 
flat.  In  modem  India  generally  it  is  too  fiat. 
In  one  set  of  scales  in  Java  it  is  too  sharp 

fNo.  94) ;  in  the  other  set  as  fiat,  as  in  India 
Nos.  74  to  79).  These  differences  probably 
pervaded  also  the  scales  of  other  countries  as 
actually  used,  but  we  know  Greece  and  Arabia 
from  theory  only.  That  the  Fifth  is  true  on 
the  bagpipe  (No.  52)  depends  apparently  on 
the  use  of  tne  drone,  which  would  prodace 
frightful  beats  if  it  were  as  much  out  of  tone 
as  in  the  other  cases  cited. 


2.  The  use  of  Tones  and  Semitones  of  about  200  and  100  cents  depends  upon 
the  Greek  tetrachordal  system  as  modified  by  Pythagorean  intonation. 


In  Zalsal's  scale  (No.  51),  and  even  in  the 
medieval  Arabic  scales,  Nos.  57  to  65  (that  is, 
omitting  the  three  Nos.  54  to  56,  which  are 
identical  with  the  Greek,  and  the  exceptional 
scales,  Nos.  68  to  72),  they  do  not  both  exist. 
In  the  Indian  scales  they  are  overridden  by 
the  system  of  22  degrees,  and  only  un- 
designedly come  close  to  our  equal  intonation. 
In  the  ruder  scales  Nos.  82  to  87  they  cannot 
be  traced.    In  Java  they  do  not  exist  actually. 


In  China  great  difficulty  was  felt  by  the  native 
musicians  of  the  Health  Exhibition  of  1884  in 
respect  to  the  Semitones.  The  Tones  were 
variable,  and  in  some  cases  there  seemed  to 
be  a  liking  for  the  minor  Tone  as  in  tuning 
the  Tamboura,  No.  108.  In  Japan  Semitones 
and  Tones  play  a  great  part  theoretically,  bat 
in  the  only  practical  cases  I  have  been  able  to 
observe,  Nos.  iii  and  112  both  were  very  un- 
certain, and  the  Fourth  was  absent. 


Digitized  by  VjOOQlC 


8BCT.  K. 


NON-HARMONIC  SCALES. 


525 


3,  Neutral  intervals,  each  lying  between  two  European  intervals,  and  having 
the  character  of  neither,  but  serving  for  either,  abound. 


The  earliest  instance  is  151  cents  in  No.  10, 
which  is  the  Threequartertone  between  the 
Semitone  of  90  or  112  cents  and  the  tone  of 
1 82  or  204  cents ;  thus  i  x  (90  +  204)  =  147  " 
^x  (112+ 182).  In  ZalzaPs,  No.  51,  it  was 
355- 204 « 151,  and  498-355 -H3  cents;  in 
the  bagpipe  observed,  No.  52,  it  was  341  — 
197  =  144,  and  495  -  341  =  1 54»  853  -  703  «= » 50» 
1009-853=146  cents,  merely  variants  of 
tuning;  Meshaqah's  theory  gave  150  cents. 
In  bagpipe  music  it  serves  indifferently  for 
what  would  be  a  Tone  or  a  Semitone  in  music 
for  another  instrument.  In  the  Japanese 
cases,  No.  iii  represents  the  two  theoretical 
Semitones  by  357- 193  =  164,  and  801  -  719=* 
82  cents,  and  No.  112  by  337-185  =  152,  and 
790— 683=  107  cents. 

Zalzal's  neutral  Third  of  355  cents,  No.  51, 
is  so  truly  neutral  between  the  just  minor  and 


major  Thirds,  316  and  386  cents,  that  Mr. 
Hipkins  was  quite  unable  to  determine  to 
which  it  most  nearly  approached  in  character, 
but  for  345  and  365  cents,  as  tried  on  my 
dichord  (p.  5^26),  the  minor  and  major 
characters  were  slightly  but  decidedly  felt. 
In  the  observed  bagpipe  this  interval  was  341 
cents.  In  Meshaq^'s  Quartertone  tempera- 
ment it  was  350  cents,  which  may  be  taken  as 
its  usual  tempered  form.  It  is  the  374  of  the 
New  Indian,  No.  74,  as  shown  in  Nos.  76,  IT^ 
and  80.  Compare  also  Nos.  81  to  84,  103, 
105,  106,  and  109.  The  correlative  neutral 
Sixth  arises  similarly. 

The  neutral  Tritone,   550  cents,   is  also  ^ 
sometimes  found,  but  it  is  rare,  and  as  the 
Tritone,  600  cents,  is  itself  rare,  this  neutral 
form  is  not  easily  observed.    See  Nos.  73,  74, 
76,  78,  79,  82,  83,  84,  and  perhaps  96. 


4.  Modem  Arabic  and  Indian  scales  have  changing  or  alternative  intervals, 
produced  by  varying  the  pitch  of  one  or  more  of  the  regular  notes  in  any  one  scale 
by  a  Quartertone  or  Degree. 


To  a  smaller  extent  alternative  tones  are 
known  in  Europe.  Thus  the  just  major  scale 
borrows  its  occasional  grave  second  from  the 
Bubdominant  key,  where  the  difference  is  only 
a  comma  of  22  cents.  Nos.  62,  63,  65  have 
each  two  tones  which  differ  by  only  a  comma 
of  24  cents,  namely  11 76  and  1200  in  Nos.  62 
and  63,  and  678  and  702  in  No.  65.  The 
scales  consequently  have  8  tones,  as  our  just 
major  scale  of  C  would  have  if  we  inserted 
both  D  and  D^,  This,  however,  disappears  in 
tempered  intonation.  Again  our  just  ascend- 
ing minor  scale  of  A^  has  two  alternative 
notes,  F^  and  G^S  ,  as  well  as  F  and  G,  and 
these  reknain  in  tempered  intonation.  If  we 
Inserted  these,  we  might  say  that  our  minor 
scale  had  9  tones.  Similarly  No.  64  has 
8  tones,  with  three  intervals  of  114  cents 
between  180  and  294,  678  and  792,  1086  and 
1200,  and  one  interval  of  90  cents  between 
792  and  882.  We  might  suppose  that  792  and 
882  are  alternative  notes,  and  that  we  might 
play  either 

o  180  294  498  678  882  1086  1200,  or  else 
o  180  294  498  678  792  1086  1200, 

the  three  final  intervals  in  the  first  case  being 
204  204  114,  and  in  the  second  114  294  114. 
This,  however,  is  only  an  illustration,  and  is 
not  the  point  raised.  Meshaqah's  complete 
scale  consists  of  24  Quartertones  to  the  Octave, 
for  two  Octaves,  each  tone  having  its  own 
individual  name.  Of  these,  only  7  are  selected 
to  form  the  normal  scale,  namely  No.  53.  But 
any  one  of  these  7  notes  may  be  raised  (or  also 
probably  depressed)  by  one  or  two  Quarter- 
tonps.    And  so  freely  is  this  variation  of  the 


scale  employed,  that  of  the  95  snatches  of 
melodies  which  Eli  Smith  reports  from  Mesha- 
qah,  there  are  only  7  in  whi(^  some  change  is 
not  occasionally  made.  Sometimes  the  change 
is  in  ascending  and  not  in  descending  or  con- 
versely. Thus  in  the  air  called  Remel,  1  find 
9  notes,  the  alternatives  950  and  11 00  being 
introduced  so  that  the  scale,  instead  of  ending 
700  850  1000  1200  ends  as  700  850  950  1000  ^ 
1 100  1200.  Something  of  the  kind  occurs  in 
bagpipe-playing  at  the  present  day,  owing  to 
the  system  known  as  *crossfingering,'  which 
gives  nominally  two  ways  of  fingering  the 
same  g"^  but  actually  produces  two  sightly 
different  notes,  the  sharper  being  used  in 
ascending  passages.  This  was  observed  by 
Mr.  Bosanquet  at  a  bagpipe  competition,  and 
has  been  confirmed  on  inquiry  by  Messrs. 
Glen,  the  great  bagpipe  makers  of  Edinburgh. 

A  similar  thing  apparently  occurs  in  In£an 
scales,  where  some  of  the  notes  may  be  de- 
pressed one,  two,  or  three  degrees,  and  others 
raised  by  similar  amounts  as  shewn  in  Nos.  73 
and  74.  And  there  seem  to  be  other  altera- 
tions of  the  kind  not  written,  but  conditioned 
by  the  rdgini  or  modelet  in  which  the 
musician  is  playing.  This  is  a  point  which 
greatly  requires  elucidation.  ^ 

These  tones  changing  by  a  degree  are  made 
by  pressing  the  string  behind  the  fret  or  de- 
flecting it  along  the  fret.  A  similar  thing 
occurs  in  playing  the  Japanese  koto.  The 
player  is  copstantly  pressing  slightly  or  heavily 
on  the  string  beyond  the  bridge,  or  pulling 
the  string  towards  the  bridge,  and  thus  more 
or  less  sharpening  or  flattening  the  pitch  of 
the  note. 


5.  Scales  of  five  tones  may  be  formed  by  omission  from  scales  of  seven  tones, 
but  on  the  other  hand  many  scales  of  five  tones  seem  to  be  entirely  independent  of 
tones  of  seven  tones,  neither  generating  them  nor  being  generated  by  them. 


Though  some  of  Chinese  pentatonic  scales, 
as  Nos.  103  and  107,  seem  to  be  derived  from 
heptatonic  or  conversely,  yet  all  the  Javese 


pentatonic  scales  are  thoroughly  independent 
of  any  heptatonic  form.  No.  94  and  Nos.  97 
to  102  could  not  be  expressed  as  parts  of  even 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


526 


ADDITIONS  BY   THE   TRANSLATOR. 


APP.  XX. 


oar  ohromatio  sc&le  of  12  semitones.  Euro- 
pean masioians,  indeed,  persist  in  hearing  and 
writing  the  Salindro  scales, 

properly  o  240  480  720  960  1200, 
as  o  200  500  700  9cx>  1200, 
or  o    300    500    700    1000    1200 

bnt  this  mast  arise  from  their  not  appreciating 
240  cents,  which  is  almost  a  neutral  intervid 
between  a  Tone,  200,  and  a  minor  Third,  300, 
and  is  hence  mistaken  by  European  ears  some- 
times for  one  and  sometimes  for  the  other. 
As  for  the  Pelog  scales,  I  cannot  find  that 
any  one  has  ventured  to  put  their  airs  into  a 
European  dress,  the  intervals  No.  96  are  so 
strange.  I  have  however  tried  to  appreciate 
them  by  having  a  concertina  tuned  with  the 
white  studs  in  Saldndro  (tuning  E  and  F  and 
^  also  B  and  C  as  unisons),  and  the  black  studs 
.  in  Pelog,  and  writing  them  witH  the  notes 


which  would  belong  to  the  stud  used  in  the 
ordinary  tuning.  For  the  Saldndro  I  used  the 
airs  in  Raffles's  Java,  and  in  Crawford's  paper 
in  the  Tagore  collection.  For  the  Pelog  I  had 
to  invent  airs  myself.  The  characters  of  the 
two  sets  are  quite  unlike.  But  pentatonio 
Scotch  airs  played  with  the  Saldndro  scale  are 
quite  recognisable.  Whether  this  scale  is  the 
primitive  pentatonio  scale  it  is  quite  impos- 
sible to  say. 

In  Japan  the  koto  timings  are  all  penta- 
tonio, and  according  to  theory  the  *  popular  * 
have  intervals  of  a  Semitone,  a  Tone,  and  a 
major  Thirds  whereas  the  *  classical '  have 
intervals  only  of  a  Tone  and  a  minor  Thirds 
just  as  on  the  commonly  received  black  digi- 
tals of  a  piano ;  but  the  classical  is  said  to 
come  from  China.  In  practice  probably  these 
intervals  are  varied  as  in  Nos.  11 1  and  112. 


6.  Pentatonio  scales  do  not  necessarily  arise  from  inability  to  appreciate 
Semitones. 


This  is  shewn  by  the  Javese  Pelog  notes, 
which  contain  intervals  of  137,  129,  112,  133, 
and  102  cents,  and  by  the  Japanese  popular 
koto  tunings,  No.  no  and  Nos.  113  to  123,  all 
pentatonio,  and  theoretically  founded  on  the 

8.  There  is  an  entire  absence  of  tonality  in  our  sense  of  the  term  and  of  any 
attempt  at  harmony. 


diatonic  Semitone.  If  in  practice  the  diatonic 
Semitone  sometimes  grows  to  a  Threeqnarter- 
tone,  it  also  sinks  to  a  small  Semitone,  see  Nos. 
Ill  and  112. 


There  is  regard  to  the  final  cadence,  at 
least  in  Meshaqah^s  scales,  and  probably  in 
all.  There  is  in  the  Indian  a  ruler  note  (vddi), 

«r  see  p.  243c',  and  minister  notes  ^am/oddij^ 
which  function  as  our  tonic,  dominant  and 
subdominant  in  certain  respects,  and  Prof. 
Helmholtz  thinks  he  discovers  a  reference  to  a 
tonic  in  Aristotle  (p.  241).  But  the  European 
feeling  of  tonality  is  one  of  very  late  growth, 
and  in  non-harmonic  scales  must  have  been 
something  quite  different,  and  if  we  refer  it  to 
the  same  feeling  as  our  own,  it  is  from  want  of 
power  to  appreciate  the  feeling  of  those  who 
use  non-harmonic  scales.  This  is  parallel  to 
what  constantly  happens  in  appreciating  the 
intervals  of  these  scales. 

There  is  plenty  of  ensemble  playing  with 
notes  of  very  different  qualities  of  tone,  but 
but  they  regularly  proceed  in  unisons  and  Oc- 
taves. In  the  Indian  instruments  there  are 
sympathetic  and  secondary  strings.  The  for- 
mer have  their  partials  evoked  by  the  notes 
played.    The  latter,  generally  tuned  in  rela- 

^  tions  of  an  Octave  Fourth  or  Fifth,  are  occa- 
sionally thrunmied.  But  there  is  nothing  like 
a  chord,  or  a  tissue  of  harmony.  It  would  not 
be  possible  with  the  notes  at  command.  There 
is  also  discant  playing  as  in  the  old  polyphony 
before  harmony  proper  was  invented.  Prof. 
Land,  speaking  of  the  Gamelan,  or  band  of 
Javese  musicians  sent  by  the  independent 
prince  of  Solo  to  the  Amheim  Industrial  Ex- 
hibition in  1879,  says :  *  The  musical  treat- 
ment is  this.  The  rab4b  plays  the  tune  in  the 
character  of  leader*  [at  the  Aquarium,  the 
player  of  the  gambang  (wooden  bar  harmo- 
nium) seemed  to  be  leader] ;  *  the  others  play 
the  same  tune,  but  figured,  and  each  for  him- 
self and  in  his  own  way ;  the  s4ron  (metal  bar 
harmonium)  resumes  the  motive  or  tune.  All 
this  is  accompanied  by  a  sort  of  basso  osti- 
7iato,  and  a  rhythmical  movement  of  the  drum. 


and  the  whole  is  divided  into  regular  sections 
and  subsections  by  the  periodical  strokes  of 
the  gongs  and  kenongs  [kettles].  The  Taria- 
tions  of  the  same  tune  by  the  different  instru- 
ments produce  a  sort  of  barbarous  harmony, 
which  has,  however,  its  lucid  moments, 
when  the  beautiful  tone  of  the  instruments 
yields  a  wonderful  effect.  But  the  principal 
charm  is  in  the  quality  of  the  sound,  and  the 
rhythmical  accuracy  of  the  playing.  The 
players  know  by  heart  a  oouple  of  hundred 
pieces,  so  as  to  be  able  to  take  any  of  the 
instruments  in  turn.' 

In  his  report  on  Japanese  music,  Mr.  S 
Isawa,  director  of  the  Musical  Institute  at 
Tokio,  distinctly  claims  a  species  of  harmony 
for  Japan,  and  gives  an  arrangement  of  the 
Greek  'Hymn  to  Apollo'  (Chappell,  p.  174), 
which  he  had  directed  'a  Ck>urt  musician, 
and  a  member  of  the  [Musical]  Institute,  to 
harmonise  purely  according  to  the  principles 
of  Japanese  classical  music'  It  was  set  for 
five  instruments,  the  Riuteki  (fuye),  Hiohiriki, 
Sho,  Koto,  and  Biwa.  I  possess  the  oopyof 
the  music  in  European  notation,  sent  to  the 
Educational  Section  of  the  Health  Exhibiti<Mi 
in  1884.  Though  much  was  in  Octaves,  the 
koto  played  a  figured  form,  with  dissonances, 
followed  by  consonances.  A  non-professionsl 
Japanese  gentleman,  a  student  of  physics,  ac- 
quainted with  European  music,  in  answering 
my  questions,  says :  *  Anything  like  Eniopean 
[harmony]  cannot  be  heard  in  Japan.  If  it 
exist,  it  is  of  the  rudest  possible  description. 
We  have  certainly  ensemble  playing  with  many 
instruments  of  different  sorts;  but  it  seems 
to  me  that  we  have  no  idea  of  such  things 
as  chords.  .  .  .  We  go  generally  parallel  in 
Octaves  and  in  Fifths,  rarely  in  Fourths, 
but  there  are  cases  where  two  different  tones, 
not  belonging  to  the  three  consonances,  are 
sounded,  but  they  are  not  harmonic^  but  what 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


SECT.L.  RECENT  WORK  ON  BEATS  AND  COMBINATIONAL  TONES,  527 

Helmholtz  calls  polyphonic.  We  have  many  they  do  not  seem  to  distinguish  from  Earo- 
figures  for  accompaniment.  ...  In  popular  pean  equally  tempered  notes,  and  which  will 
music,  we  meet  with  cases  where  two  instru-  probably  be  soon  reduced  to  that  form  by  the 
ments  play  Octaves  or  Fifths.  With  singing  labour  of  Mr.  Isawa,  there  seems  to  be  no 
this  would  also  hold,  but  it  is  very  rare  that  reason  why  harmony  should  not  be  natural- 
people  ever  sing  chorus.'  ised,  like  so  many  European  customs,  in  the 

At  the  same  time,  as  the  Japanese  use  a  wonderfully  progressive  country  of  Japan, 
system  of  twelve  notes  to  the  Octave,  which 

It  may  be  added,  although  it  cannot  appear  from  the  table  of  the  scales,  that  in 
listening  to  native  Javese,  Chinese,  and  Japanese  performers,  there  seemed  to  be 
a  total  absence  of  what  we  term  expression.  There  was  no  piano  and  forte,  no 
shading  or  nuance,  merely  a  hard  playing  of  the  notes,  as  on  street  mechanical 
pianos.  They  appeared  to  depend  principally  on  gongs,  clacks,  or  accumulation 
of  various  instruments  to  give  rhythm  and  spirit  to  the  music.  But  so  far  as  I 
could  judge  by  the  very  little  India  music  I  heard  from  Bajah  K4m  P41  Singh,  it 
seems  to  have  some  expression,  as  it  certainly  has  an  extremely  varied  rhythm, 
sounding  very  strange  to  European  ears.    (See  Siamese  scales,  Postscript,  p.  556.)  H 


SECTION  L. 

BECENT  WORK  ON   BEATS  AND  COMBINATIONAL  TONES. 

(See  notes  throughout  Part  II.,  pp.  152-233,  and  especially  pp.  43,  55,  126,  151,  152,  155,  156, 
157,  159,  167,  199,  202,  204,  205,  226,  229,  231,  and  420.  The  reader  is  particularly  re- 
quested to  defer  any  reference  to  this  Section  L  until  he  has  studied  Part  II.,  and  become 
familiar  with  the  whole  phenomenon  of  beats  and  combinational  tones,  and  with  Prof. 
Helmholtz*s  theories  respecting  their  origin.    Until  such  familiarity  has  been  gained,  much 

*     of  what  follows  will  be  unintelligible.) 


Art. 

1.  Papers  considered,  p.  527. 

i.-v.  Koenig;    yi.  Bosanquet;    yii.   and 
viii.  Preyer. 

2.  Koenig's  Simple  Tones,  p.  528. 

(a)  Simple  tones  of  forks,  p.  528. 

(5)  Simple   tones    of   the    wave-sirens, 

p.  529. 

3.  The  Phenomena   which  arise  when  Two 

Tones  are  Sounded  together,  p.  529. 
(a)  The  facts  as  distinct  from  theory, 
p.  529. 

(5)  Upper  and    lower  beats  and  beat- 

notes,  p.  529. 

(c)  Limits  within  which  either  one  or 

both  beat-notes  are  heard,  p.  529. 

(d)  Beat-notes    and    differential    tones, 

p.  530.  ,    ^       ^ 

(0)  Bosanquet's  summary  of  the  phe- 
nomena, p.  530. 

4.  Objective  Beats  and  Subjective  Beats,  Beat- 

Notes,  and  Differential  Tones,  p.  531. 
{a)  Objective  beats,  531. 

(6)  Subjective  beats  and  notes,  p.  531. 
(c)  Preyer*s  experiments    to  shew    the 

subjectivity  of  differential  tones, 
p.  531. 


(d)  Subjectivity  of  Summational  Tones, 
p.  532. 
Theory  of  Beats,  Beat-Notes,  and  Combi- 
national Tones,  p.  532. 
{a\  Origin  of  beats,  p.  532.  % 

\h)  Can  beats  generate  tones?     First, 
beats  of  intermittence,  p.  533. 

(c)  Can  beats  generate  tones  ?    Secondly, 

beats  of  interference,  p.  533. 

(d)  Would  a  tone  generated  by  beats  be 

louder  than  its  primaries  ?  p.  534. 

(e)  Experiments    with    the    wave-siren, 

p.  534. 
(/)  Beats  and  beat-notes  heard  together, 

P-  535- 
(g)  Beat-notes  and  beat-tones,  p.  535. 
0i)  Koenig' s  explanation  of  summational 

tones,  p.  536. 
(i)  Eoenig*s    theory    of    the    origin    of 

beat-notes,  p.  536. 
(k)  Lecture-room  demonstration  of  beat- 
notes,  p.  536. 
Influence  of  Difference  of  Phase  on  Quality     . 
of  Tone,  p.  537.  ^ 

Influence  of  Combinational  Tones  on  the 
Consonance  of  Simple  Tones,  p.  537. 


Art  I.  Papers  considered. 
The  papers  here  considered  are 

i.  B.  Koenig.  Ueher  den  Ztcsammenklang  Zweier  Tone  (On  the  sounding  of 
two  tones  at  the  same  time).    Pogg.  Annal.  Feb.  1876,  vol.  157,  pp.  177-237. 

This  paper  appeared  a  year  before  the  4th 
German  option  of  Helmholtz's  Tonempfin' 
dun§en,  and  is  cursorily  referred  to,  supriL,  p. 
1596.  The  other  papers  of  Koenig  here  men- 
tioned appeared  subsequent  to  Prof.  Helm- 
holtz*s  4th  German  edition.  But  this  paper 
is  placed  first  because  it  commenced  the  new 
investigations.  A  translation  appeared  in  the 
Philosophical  Magazine,  June  1876,  and  sup- 
plement of  the  same  date,  pp.  417-446,  51 1- 


525,  under  the  title  *  On  the  Simultaneous 
Sounding  of  Two  Notes,*  and  communicated 
by  the  late  W.  Spottiswoode,  President  R.  S., 
who  also  read  a  paper  on  '  Beats  and  Combi- 
nation Tones  *  before  the  Musical  Association 
on  May  5,  1879  {Proceedings  of  Mas.  A., 
1878-9,  pp.  1 18-130),  when  he  exhibited  K.'s 
apparatus  and  repeated  several  of  his  experi- 
ments. 


Digitized  by 


Google 


528  ADDITIONS  BY  THE   TRANSLATOK.  app.  xi. 

ii.  R.  KoENiG.  Ueher  die  Erregung  harmonischer  Ohertone  durch  Sckwingungen 
eines  Grundtones  (On  the  excitement  of  harmonic  upper  partials  hy  the  vibrations 
of  a  fundamental  tone).     Wiedemann,  AnnaL  1880,  vol.  xi.,  pp.  857-870. 

This  treats  of  a  subject  incidentally  mentioned,  soprA,  p.  159a,  in  reference  to  combinational 
tones. 

iii.  R.  KoENiQ.  Ueher  den  Ursprung  der  Stosse  und  Stosstone  bei  harmonischen 
Intervallen  (On  the  origin  of .  beats  and  beat-tones  for  harmonic  intervals). 
Wiedemann,  Vnnal.  1881,  vol.  xii.  pp.  335-349,  introducing  an  entirely  new 
method  of  experimenting  by  means  of  the  wave-siren. 

iv.  R.  KoENiG.  Beschreibung  einss  Stosstoneapparates  fur  Vorlesungsversucke 
(Description  of  a  beat-tone  apparatus  for  lecture-room  experiments),  Wiedem.,  1881, 
immediately  after  the  last  paper,  vol.  xii.  pp.  350-353. 

V.  R.  KoENiG.  Bemerhungen  iiber  die  Klangfarbe  (Remarks  on  quality  of  tone) 
^  Wied.,  1 88 1,  vol.  xiv.,  pp.  369-393,  more  folly  describing  the  wave  siren  of  No.  iii. 

In  drawing  up  this  notice  I  made  use  solely  p^riences  d'AcoustiquSj  1882,  to  be  had  at  his 

of  the  original  Q«rman  papers  just  cited.    But  present  establishment,  27  Quai  d'Anjou,  Paris, 

I  find  that  Dr.  Koenig  has  republished  the  and  I  have  made  some  use  of  the  additional 

whole  of  his  16  acoustical  papers,  of  which  notes  then    added.    I    cordially    recommend 

those  just  cited  form  the  9th,  14th,  loth,  nth,  this  collection  as  a  valuable  and  almost  indis- 

and  1 6th,  respectively,  in  the  French  language,  pensable   supplement    to    Prof.    Helmholtz's 

in  one  volume,  with  beautifully  printed  wood-  work, 
engravings,  under  the  title  of  Quelques  Ex- 

vi.  R.  H.  M.  BoBANQUBT.  On  the  Beats  of  Consonances  of  the  Form  h:  i. 
Proceedings  of  the  Physical  Society  of  London,  vol.  iv.,  Aug.  1880  to  Dec.  1881, 
pp.  221-256.     This  was  written  before  B.  had  seen  No.  iiL  and  iv.  above.  • 

vii.  W.  Preyee.  Ueber  die  Orenzen  der  Tonwahmehmung  (On  the  limits 
of  the  perception  of  tone)  containing  the  sections.  I.  The  Lower  Tones.  IL  The 
Highest  Tones.  IIL  Sensitiveness  for  Difference  of  Pitch.  IV.  Sensitiveness 
^  for  the  Sensation  of  Interval.  V.  Sensation  of  Silence.  Forming  the  first  part 
of  the  first  series  of  Physiologische  Abhandlungen  (Physiological  Essays)  edited 
by  W.  Preyer,  M.D.  and  Ph.D.,  Prof,  of  Physiology  and  Director  of  the  Physio- 
logical Institute  at  Jena,  1876,  the  year  before  the  publication  of  the  4th  German 
edition  of  Hehnholtz,  who  quotes  it  several  times.  It  is  here  inserted  for  com- 
pleteness. 

viiL  W.  Pbeyeb.  Akustische  Untersuchungen  (Acoustical  Investigations)  in  tlie 
same  collection,  second  series,  fourth  part,  containing  I.  Deepest  Tones  without 
upper  partial  Tones  (supri,  see  footnote,  pp.  176-7).  II.  Combinational  Tones  and 
upper  partial  Tones  of  Tuning  Forks.  EDL  Contributions  to  the  Theory  of  Con- 
sonance. IV.  Notice  on  the  Perception  of  the  smallest  differences  of  Pitch, 
Jena,  1879. 

These  will  be  cited  by  the  initial  of  the  editions  of  vii.  and  viii.    Prof.  Hehnholtz  will 

author,  E.  or  B.  or  P.  followed  by  the  number  be  cited  as  H.,  generally  followed  by  tiie  page 

of  the  paper,  and  generally  by  the  page,  which  of  this  edition, 
in  the  case  of  P.  will  refer  to  the  separate 

^        Art.  2.  Koenig' s  Simple  Tones, 

(a)  Simple  Tones  of  Forks,  The  tones  dealt  with  by  K.  are  as  simple  as 
K  could  make  them.  *  The  forks  that  I  used  with  resonators,'  says  K.  iii.  337, 
*  had  no  recognisable  harmonic  upper  partials  at  all.  The  occurrence  of  harmonic 
upper  partials  in  tuning-forks  depends  not  so  much  on  the  lowness  of  their  piteli 
and  the  amplitude  of  their  \ibrations  as  on  the  relation  of  the  amplitude  to  the 
thickness  of  the  prongs.' 

From  a  c  fork  (128  d.  v.)   with  prongs  opening  of  the  resonator  tuned  to  them,  almost 

7  mm.  ( « '28  inch)  thick,  K.  obtained  as  many  touched  the  prongs  of  the  fork.    The  pitch  of 

as  4  partials.     From    another  c  fork  with  forks  varies  directly  as  the  thickness,  and  in- 

prongs  15  mm.  (»'59  inch)  thick  and  20  mm.  versely  as  the  square  of  the  length,  of  their 

(s  79  inch)  wide,  only  2  partials  were  gene-  prongs  (K.  iii.  338).    K.  proceeds  to  mention 

rally  obtained,  but  extremely  violent  blows  that  the  forks  he  used,  even  the  largest,  when 

brought    out    a    3rd    partial.     With    prongs  placed  before  properly  tuned  resonators,  had  no 

29  mm.  (1*14  inch)  thick  and  40  mm.  (=1-57  detectable  upper  partials.     Subsequently  B. 

inch)  wide,  it  was  not  possible  to  hear  even  a  repeated  K.'s  observations  in  part  with  the 

faint  Octave,  and  a  Twelfth,  except  when  the  stopped    organ-pipes    of     B.'s    experimental 


Digitized  by  V^OOQIC 


MOT.  L.  RECENT  WORK  ON  BEATS  &  COMBINATIONAL  TONES-  529 


organ,  in  which  only  the  Twelfth  or  3rd  par- 
tial was  perceptible  and  could  be  allowed  for. 
Also,  afterwards,  K.  iii.  342,  used  stopped 
organ-pipes  and  tuning-forks.    B.  used  tones 

(6)  Simple  Tones  of  the  Wave  Siren. 
however,  E.  invented  the  wave  siren. 

An  harmonic  curve  constructed  on  a  large 
scale  and  reduced  by  photography  was  cut  on 
the  edge  of  a  wheel.  The  wheel  revolved 
under  a  narrow  slit,  placed  exactly  in  the 
position  of  a  radius  of  the  wheel,  through 
which  wind  was  driven  as  the  wheel  rotated. 
The  curve  alternately  cut  oS.  and  let  pass  the 
stream  of  air,  and  produced  a  perfectly  simple 
tone,  the  pitch  of  which  depended  on  the  ra- 
pidity of  rotation.  Forms  of  this  wave  siren 
are  figured  in  E.  iii.  346,  347,  and  E.  v.  386. 
The  last  shews  16  harmonic  curves  which 
may  be  made  to  act  in  any  groups,  producing 
all  the  combinations  of  perfectly  simple  tones, 


of  moderate  force,  and  E.  also  used  weak  tones 
with  the  pipe,  and  not  the  strong  tones  of  his 
tuning-forks  mentioned  in  E.  i. 

To  avoid  the  suspicion  of  upper  partials, 

of  which  the  ratio  numbers  lie  between  i  and 
16.  Hence,  although  upper  partials  are  found 
on  most  tuning-forks,  and  especially  on  cer- 
tain of  E.'s  forks,  it  would  be  wrong  to  assume 
(as  P.  ii.  38  apparently  assumes)  that  in  all 
E.'s  cases,  at  least  the  Octave  was  audible. 
This  waa  not  the  case  with  stopped  organ - 
pipes  used  by  both  E.  and  B.,  and  still  less  so 
with  the  tones  of  the  wave  siren.  E.'s  results  • 
therefore  cannot  be  explained  by  upper  partial 
tones.  But  when  we  are  dealing  with  com- 
pound  tones,  each  pair  of  partials  forms  a  li 
combination  of  simple  tones  to  which  E.'s 
observations  apply. 


'Art.  3.  The  Phenomena  which  arise  when  two  notes  are  sounded  together, 
according  to  Koenig  and  Bosanquet, 

(a)  The  facts  as  distinct  from  theory.  We  must  distinguish  the  phenomena  from 
any  theoretical  explanation  of  them  that  may  be  proposed.  The  phenomena  de- 
scribed by  such  an  acoustician  as  K.,  so  careful  in  experiments,  so  amply  provided 
with  the  most  exact  instruments,  will,  I  presume,  be  generally  accepted.  The 
theory  by  which  he  seeks  to  account  for  them  is  a  matter  for  discussion.  The 
following  relates  to  two  simple  tones  only,  and  this  must  be  carefully  borne  in 
mind,  because  H.  1592^  apparently  imagined  that  the  tones  used  really  had  upper 
partials. 

[h)    Upper  and  Lower  Beats  and  Beat-notes, 

If  two  simple  tones  of  either  very  slightly  or  greatly  different  pitches,  called 
generators,  be  sounded  together,  then  the  upper  pitch  number  necessarily  Hes  ^r 
between  two  multiples  of  the  lower  pitch  number,  one  smaller  and  the  other 
greater,  and  the  differences  between  these  multiples  of  the  pitch  number  of  the 
lower  generator  and  the  pitch  number  of  the  upper  generator  give  two  numbers 
which  either  determine  the  frequency  of  the  two  sets  of  beats  which  may  be  heard 
or  the  pitch  of  the  two  beat-notes  which  may  be  heard  in  their  place.  The  term 
*  beat-notes  '  is  here  used  without  any  theory  as  the  origin  of  stwh  tones^  but  only 
to  shew  that  they  are  tones  having  the  same  frequency  as  the  beats,  which  are 
sometimes  heard  simultaneously. 

Beferring  to  the  tables  in  the  Translator's 
footnote  to  p.  191  suprd,  which  relate  to  com- 
pound tones,  and  therefore  contain  multiples 
of  the  pitch  numbers  (or  of  the  numbers  which 
give  the  interval  ratios)  of  two  generators,  we 
see  from  the  minor  Tenth' 5  :  12,  that  the 
prime  12  of  the  upper  generator  lies  between 
10  and  15,  the  2nd  and  3rd  multiples  of  the 
lower  generator,  and  hence  the  beat  or  beat- 
note  frequencies  would  be  12— 10=  2,  and 
15  — 12  =  3.  If,  then,  the  two  generators  are 
low  enough,  say,  having  the  pitch-numbers 
.  5  X  6  »  30  and  12  x  6  =  72,  the  beats  heard 
would  be  2  X  6  »  12  and  3  x  6  »  18,  which 


But  if  they  were  higher,  as  5  x  20  «  100  and 
12  X  20  «  240,  the  beats  would  be  2  x  20  »  40, 
and  3  X  20  —  60,  which,  though  far  too  rapid 
to  be  counted,  would  be  clearly  heard  as  beats, 
and  at  the  sanie  titne  the  beat-notes  of  40  and 
60  vib.  would  aUo  be  audible.  If,  however, 
they  were  much  higher,  as  5  x  100  »  500  and 
12  X  100  =  1200,  then  only  the  beat-notes  of 
2  X  100  =  200  and  3  X  100  =  300  vib.  would  ir 
be  heard.  The  beats  heard  are  then  the  same 
as  if  the  upper  generator  were  simple  and  the 
lower  generator  compound;  but  it  must  be 
remembered  that  both  generators  are  really 
simple. 


would    be  plainly  distinguishable   as    beats. 

The  frequency  arising  from  the  lower  multiple  of  the  lower  generator  is  called  ' 
the  frequency  of  the  lower  beat  or  lower  beat-note,  that  arising  from  the  higher 
multiple  is  called  the  frequency  of  the  higher  beat  or  beat-note,  without  at  aU 
implying  that  one  set  of  beats  should  be  greater  or  less  than  the  other,  or  that 
one  beat-note  should  be  sharper  or  flatter  than  the  other.  They  are  in  reality 
sometimes  one  way  and  sometimes  the  other.  < 

(c)  Limits  within  lohich  either  one  or  both  Beat-Notes  are  heard.  Both  sets  of 
beats,  or  both  beat-notes,  are  not  usually  heard  at  the  same  time.  If  we  divide  the 
intervals  examined  into  groups  (i)  from  1:1  to  i  :  2,  (2)  from  i  :  2  to  i  :  3, 
(3)  from  I  :  3  to  I  :  4,  (4)  from  i  :  4  to  1:5,  and  so  on,  the  lower  beats  and 

Digitized  by  VuiODQ  IC 


530 


ADDITIONS  BY  THE  TRANSLATOR. 


APP.  XX. 


beat-tones  extend  over  little  more  than  the  lower  half  of  each  group,  and  the 
upper  beats  and  beat- tones  over  little  more  than  the  upper  half.  For  a  short 
distance  in  the  middle  of  each  period  both  sets  of  beats,  or  both  beat-notes,  are 
audible,  and  these  beat-notes  beat  with  each  other,  forming  secondary  beats,  or  are 
replaced  by  new  or  secondary  beat-notes. 

(d)  Beat-Notes  and  Differential  Tones,  The  lower  beats,  as  long  as  they  are 
distinctly  audible,  and  refer  to  an  interval  less  than  5  :  6,  or  a  minor  Third,  sgree 
with  the  beats  of  H.  171a,  and  when  they  have  a  greater  frequency  than  from  16 
to  20  there  is  also  heard  the  beat-note,  which  then  coincides  in  pitch  with  the 
differential  tone  of  H.  153a.  Above  a  minor  Third,  H.  ijid  says  the  beats  are 
practically  inaudible.  K.  however  hears  them — and  B.  vi.  235-237  also  heard 
them — passing  over  into  a  roll  and  a  confused  rattle,  as  far  as  the  major  Sixth 
(G  :  Ai){or  the  lower  beats. 

confased  when  hoard  separately,  althongh  the 
frequency  was  the  same.  K.,  so  far  as  I  can 
see,  does  not  anywhere  mention  the  pitch  of 
the  beats  he  heard,  bat  B.,  tI.  237-9.  sajs 
that  in  all  cases  he  has  obserred,  when  the 
required  partials  have  been  remoyed,  'the 
beats  .  .  .  consist  entirely  of  variations  of  in- 
tensity of  the  lower  note/  and  adds  that,  *  as 
he  (E.)  does  not  analyse  the  beats,  we  cannot 
tell  whether  the  variations  of  the  lower  note 
were  produced  in  his  experiments.'  Mr.  Blaik- 
ley  {Proc,  Mub,  Asm,  i8Sf-2,  p.  25)  relates, 
however,  that  when  E.  exhibited  the  beats  to 
him  in  Paris,  E.  said  :  •  You  hear  distinctly — 
there  can  be  no  doubt  about  it— that  the  beat- 
ing note  is  the  lower  one.'  This  gives  K.*s 
opinion,  which  Mr.  Blaikley  did  not   share. 

Observe  that  E.  does  not  deny  the  exist- 
ence of  tones  having  the  pitch  of  differential 
(or  summational)  tones,  but,  as  in  this  case, 
he  shews  their  existence,  and  that  they  are 
distinct  from  his  beat-notes,  having  frequently 
a  different,  and  only  occasionally  the  same, 
pitch.  When,  therefore,  B.,  vi.  239,  talks  of 
second,  third,  and  fourth  combinational  tones 
having  been  demonstrated  directly  by  K.,  he 
seems  to  have  identified  beat-notes  and  diffe- 
rential tones,  which,  however,  E.  distinguishes. 

The  above  observations  of  E.  on  differential 
tones  are  taken  from  the  German  edition  of 
E.'s  paper  in  1876.  In  the  French  repablica- 
tion  in  1882  they  still  appear,  but  in  paren- 
theses, and  with  a  long  note  (ibid.  p.  130),  in 
which  he  states  that  subsequent  investigations 
have  induced  him  to  change  his  opinion,  as  he 
finds  that  even  very  wide  harmonic  intervals 
between  extremely  weak  tones  may  prodace 
distinct  beats.  Hence  (for  the  case  where  an 
auxiliary  fork  produced  beats  with  two  forks 
having  the  ratio  8  :  15,  shewing  a  very  weak 
tone  7,  that  might  have  arisen  from  '  the  tone 
of  the  lower  beats  of  8  and  15  '),  in  his  Ger- 
man summary  of  results,  E.  i.  236,  parai^raph 
III.  6,  admitted  the  actual  existence  of  differ- 
ential tones,  though  *  extraordinarily  weaker 
than  beat-notes ;  *  but  in  his  French  republica- 
tion (p.  147)  he  has  altered  this  paragraph  to : 
*  No  experiment  has  yet  proved  with  certainty 
the  existence  of  differential  and  summationiU 
tones.*  Observe  that  the  existence  of  tones  . 
with  the  pitch  of  differential  tones  is  not  dis-  1 
puted.  It  is  only  the  theoretical  origin  of  snch  1 
tones  that  is  called  in  question.  At  present  it  \ 
seems  impossible  to  decide  that  point. 


In  other  respects  E.'s  beat-notes  are  differ- 
^  ent  from  H.'s  differential  tones.  Thus  for  the 
minor  Tenth  5  :  12,  our  first  example,  the 
beat-notes  are  2  and  3,  as  just  shewn,  but  the 
differential  tone  is  i2-5»7,  which  is  not 
obtained  by  E.  i.  216,  who  says  :  *  These  inter- 
vals, which  are  formed  by  high  tones,  allow 
the  beat-notes  to  be  heard  quite  loudly,  but 
give  no  trace  of  differential  tones.  Thus 
&"  :  b'"  (8  :  15)  gives  only  i  and  no  trace  of  7, 
c'"  :  d'"  (4  :  9)  gives  only  i  and  nothing  of 
e"'  (5) ;  &"  :r  (3  :  8)  only  /  and  /'  and  no 
a*"  (5)  at  all,  hence  the  differential  tones  must 
be  extraordinarily  weaker  than  the  beat-notes. 
But  I  was  able  to  establish  the  actual  exist- 
ence of  these  differential  tones  with  certainty 
by  forming  the  above  intervals  with  deeper 
notes,  which,  lasting  longer,  allowed  me  by 
means  of  auxiliary  forks  to  get  a  definite 
number  of  beats  with  the  differential  tones  in 
«r  question.'  This  experiment  I  have  repeated 
.several  times.  I  made  the  tone  of  the  generat- 
ing forks  as  loud  as  possible  by  holding  them 
over  resonance  jars.  The  auxiliary  fork  had 
to  be  held  at  a  considerable  distance  from  its 
jar  in  order  to  reduce  its  loudness  to  about 
that  of  the  differential  tone,  to  allow  the  beats 
to  be  counted.  Thus  the  mistuned  minor 
Tenth  22377  :  539*18  gave  the  differential 
tone  315*41,  which,  although  inaudible,  beat 
with  the  fork  319*59  audibly  4*18,  which  was 
counted  as  4*2.    And  so  on  in  other  cases. 

In  the  case  of  a  mistuned  Octave,  it  appears 
to  me  that  the  lower  fork  acts  as  this  auxiliary 
fork  to  catch  the  differential  tone.  Thus  the 
mistuned  Octave  22377  :  451*14  gives  the  dif- 
ferential tone  227*37,  which  would  possibly 
have  been  quite  inaudible  if  it  had  not  been 
caught  by  the  lower  fork,  with  which  it  made 
^  3*6  beats  in  a  second,  as  I  myself  counted.  In 
this  case  I  had  to  hold  the  higher  fork  far 
above  the  resonance  jar.  The  beats  were  heard 
as  low  beats  at  the  pitch  of  the  lower  fork.  Also 
in  this  case,  on  continuing  to  hold  the  high  fork 
over  the  resonance  jar  of  the  upper  fork  to 
weaken  its  sound,  but  bringing  the  low  fork 
over  the  higher  resonance  jar  as  closely  as 
possible,  the  higher  Octave  of  that  fork,  or 
447'54,  was  produced,  which  beat  with  the 
higher  fork  also  3*6  times ;  but  now  the  beat 
was  clearly  and  distinctly  at  the  pitch  of  the 
npper  fork.  This  was  a  beat  of  the  2nd  par- 
tial of  the  lower  fork  with  the  upper  fork,  and 
was  altogether  distinct  in  character  from  the 
lower  beat.    Hence  the  beats  could  not  be 


ie)  BosanqneVs  summary  of  the  pJienom^na,    B.  vi.  228.     *  As  two  notes  of 
equal  amplitudes  separate  from  unison,  they  are  at  first  received  by  the  ear  in  the 

Digitized  by  V^OOQIC 


BECT.  ir.    EECENT  WOBK  ON  BEATS  &  COMBINATIONAL  TONES.    531 

manner  of  resultant  displacements,  consisting  of  the  beats  of  a  note  whose  fre- 
quency is  midway  between  the  primaries.  When  the  interval  reaches  about  two 
commas  [say  43  or  50  cents],  the  ear  begins  to  resolve  the  resultant  displacements, 
and  the  primary  notes  step  in  beside  the  beats.  When  the  interval  reaches  a 
minor  Third  in  the  ordinary  pans  of  the  scale,  neither  the  beats  nor  the  inter- 
mediate pitch  of  the  resultant  note  are  any  longer  audible,  at  least  as  matter  of 
ordinary  perception ;  but  the  resultant  displacement  which  reaches  the  ear  is 
decomposed,  and  produces  the  sensation  of  the  two  primary  notes,  perfectly  distinct 
from  each  other :  that  is  to  say,  Ohm's  law  has  set  in,  and  is  true,  for  ordinary 
perceptions  and  in  the  ordinary  regions  of  the  scale,  for  the  minor  Third  and  all 
greater  intervals.'  These  phenomena  are  not  mentioned  by  Koenig,  and  in  my 
own  observations  I  feel  a  difficulty  in  appreciating  them. 

Art.  4. — Objective  Beats  and  Subjective  Beats,  Beat-Notes  and  Differential 
Tones. 

(a)  Objective  Beats.    Beats  of  a  disturbed  unison  exist  objectively  as  disturb- 
ances in  the  air  before  it  reaches  the  ear.     They  are  reinforced  by  resonators,  the}-  % 
disturb  sand,  &c.     In  the  case  of  the  beats  of  harmonium  reeds  in  Appunn's 
tonometer,  they  strongly  shook  the  box  containing  the  reeds.     Other  beats,  beat- 
notes,  and  combinational  tones  appear  not  to  exist  externally  to  the  ear. 

(b)  Subjective  Beats  and  Notes.  K.  i.  221  says  :  *  Neither  these  combinational 
tones  nor  the  beat-notes  already  described  are  reinforced  by  resonators.*  B.  vi. 
233-4,  after  describing  his  improved  resonator,  by  means  of  which  he  can  effec- 
tually block  up  both  ears  against  any  sound  but  that  coming  from  a  resonance 
jar  (see  p.  4^d\  note  J),  says :  *  By  means  of  these  arrangements  I  some  time  ago 
examined  the  nature  of  the  ordinary  first  difference-tone,  and  convinced  myself 
that  it  is  not  capable  of  exciting  a  resonator.  In  short,  the  difference-tone  of  H., 
or  first  [lower]  beat-note  of  K.,  as  ordinarily  heard,  is  not  objective  in  its  character. 
.  .  .  When  the  nipples  of  the  resonator-attachment  fitted  tightly  into  the  ears,  no- 
thing reached  the  ear  but  the  uniform  vibrations  of  the  resonator  sounding  C.  But 
if  there  was  the  slightest  looseness  between  the  nipple  and  the  passage  of  either  ear, 
the  second  note  (c)  of  the  combination  got  in,  and  gave  rise  to  the  subjective 
difference-tone  (first  [lower]  beat-note  of  K.),  by  the  interference  of  which  with  the  ^ 
C  1  explain  the  beats  on  that  note.     These  beats  are  therefore  subjective.' 

This  expression  is  not  meant  to  imply  that  again  says,  H.  216a,  that  he  has  *  always  been 

they  are  the  prodnct  of  the  imagination,  but  able  to  hear  thefleeper  combinational  tones  o! 

that  they  do  not  exist  externally  to  the  ear.  the  second  order,  when  the  tones  have  been 

Hence,  when  H.  157c  says  that  they  are  at  played  on  the  harmonium  and  the  ear  was 

least  partly  objective,  although  he  admits  that  assisted  by  proper  resonators,'  he  had  possibly 

the  greater  part  of  the  strength  of  combina-  not  succeeded  in  blocking  both  ears  properly 

tional  tones  arises  only  within  the  ear,  and  against  the  outer  air. 

{c)  Preyer's  experiments  to  shew  the  subjectivity  of  Differential  Tones. — P.  viii. 
II.  had  seven  tuning  forks  of  extraordinary  delicacy  constructed,  giving /lyof, 
c'  256,  /'  341^,  a'  426I,  c"  S^^iJ["  6S23,  gr"  768  vibrations,  and  hence  having  the 
ratios  2:3:4:5:6:8:9,  which  were  so  ready  to  vibrate  on  the  slightest  ex- 
citement that  they  could  be  experimented  on  at  night  only.  The  three  lowest 
forks  had  the  following  partials. 

Fork  /  had  the  2nd/'  strong,  the  3rd  &'  strong,  and  the  4th/"  weak. 

Fork  c'  had  the  2nd  &'  strong  and  the  3rd  g^'  strong.  ^ 

Fork/'  had  the  2nd/"  strong. 

Sounding  these  forks  in  pairs  to  get  the  differential  tones, 

c"  &/gave/'  or  6  —  2=4  ;/"  &/gave  c"  or  8  —  2=6;  g'^  &  c' gave  c"  or  9—3=6; 

and  that  these  tones  were  objective  enough  was  shewn  by  their  making  the  forks 
/',  c"  vibrate  sympathetically.     But  we  see  that/'  and  c"  are  partials  of  /  and  c', 
which  existed  already  strongly  on  those  forks,  and  if  the  forks  /  and  &  were 
sounded  separately,   they  also  made  the  forks  /',   c"  vibrate  sjrmpathetically. 
Hence  these  results  did  not  prove  the  objective  existence  of  their  differential  dupli- 
cates.   On  the  other  hand,  the  pairs  of  forks  gi\dng  the  audible  differential  tones  — 
/"-c"  =  / or  8-6  =  2,  a'-c'  =  /or5-3  =  2,  c"-/'=/ or  6-4  =  2, 
g'  -c"  =  c' or  9-6  =  3,   a'-f=  c' or  5-2  =3,  /"-a'=c' or  8-5  =  3, 
gF"-a'=/'or9-5=4,  gf"-/  =  a' or  9-4  =  5,  /"-c'=a' or  8-3  =  5, 

utterly  failed  to  produce  the  slightest  effect  on  the  forks  having  the  same  pitch,      t 

Digitized  bykJaQUQlC 


532  ADDITIONS  BY  THE  TRANSLATOR.  app.xx. 

(d)  Subjectivity  of  Summational  Tones.  Again,  for  summational  tones  the 
combined  forks 

/+/'=c''or2  +  4=:6,  /+c"=/"  or  2  +  6  =  8,  c' +c"  =  5f"  or  3  +  6  =  9 
gave  tones  perfectly  objective,  but  then  these  tones  c",  /",  gf"  ahready  existed  as 
partials  of  one  of  the  two  forks  excited.    On  the  other  hand, 

/+c'=a' or  2  +  3  =  5,  c'+a'=/"  or  3  +  5  =  8,  /'+a'  =  sf"  or  4  +  5  =  9 

were  inaudible,  that  is,  neither  existed  without  nor  within  the  ear.  *  Perhaps,' 
says  P.,  *  they  might  be  made  audible  after  properly  arming  the  forks  by  means  of 
resonance  boxes  while  sounding.  But  the  observation  would  not  be  easy.*  Just 
as  H.  could  hear  the  cases  he  cites  (Pogg.  Ann.  vol.  xc.  1856,  p.  519)  '  only  with 
great  difficulty.'  But  the  forks  tried  by  him  each  possessed  the  Octave,  as  he 
states  (ibid.  pp.  506,  510). 

Now,  when  Octaves  exist,  and  in  case  of  P.,  *  even  the  oomprehensive  investigations  of 
€r  the  siren  other  partials  are  strongly  developed,  Koenig  do  not  make  the  [external  objective] 
these  summational  tones  —  as  G.  Appunn  existence  ^of  summationiA  tones  probable/ 
pointed  out  to  P.— could  be  conceived  as  differ-  Hence,  like  the  differential  tones,  they  most 
ential  tones  of  the  second  order— that  is,  be  generated  within  the  ear. 
differential  tones  arising  from  the  first  differ-  E.,  in  the  French  edition  of  his  papers 
ential  acting  on  the  partial,  if  such  action  is  (note,  p.  127),  says :  '  This  explanation  is  not 
admitted.  Thus  H.  found  b  +/  « (2  or  2  +  3  <=  5,  admissible,  because  it  assumes  that  two  sounds 
but  then  b  and/  included  the  Octaves  2>'and/'  always  generate  a  differential  tone,  which  is 
or  4  and  6,  and  we  had,  the  first  differential  not  correct.  For  example,  take  two  tones  cor- 
/— 6  =  Bor3  —  2  =  1,  and  the  second  f'—B  =  d"  responding  to  the  fundamental  tones  d  and  t' 
or  6—  I  =:  5,  or,  without  using  B  in  the  formula,  [my  notation],  or  256  :  320=54  :  5.  They  give 
/'—(/— 6) « d"  or  6  —  (3 - 2)  =  5.  In  this  way  the  beat-note  cf-i\  but  this  sound  I  does  not 
P.  proceeds  to  shew  that  all  the  cases  recorded  form  the  sound  7  with  the  Octave  of  c^.that  is, 
can  be  explained.  Hence  he  concludes  that  with  c"  =  8 ;  nor  does  it  form  the  sound  9  with 
the  summational  tone,  if  not  existing  as  a  the  Octave  of  e/  or  e/' » 10,  as  we  can  be  con- 
partial  on  one  of  the  tones,  is  entirely  gene-  vinced  by  sounding  at  the  same  time  the 
rated  within  the  ear.  Thus,  according  to  primary  sounds  C  and  c"->i  :  8,  or  G  and  «/'• 
K.,  i.  220,  from  c'  :  ^  =  2  :  3  he  heard  clearly  » i  :  10,  even  when  these  latter  are  much 
5  =  tf",  7=sfl"  +  c',  8  =  e"  +  gf',  9*d"',  io  =  e'"  stronger  than  the  beat-note  in  question  and 
m  and  II,  the  last  by  auxiliary  forks  which  beat  than  the  two  Octaves  of  the  primary  sounds 
with  the  required  tones.  But  there  were  in  cf  and  e/.'  The  explanation  by  differentials  of 
this  case  the  partials  2,  4,  6,  8  =  c',  c",  ^',  e'",  the  second  order  given  by  P.  is  an  adoption  of 
and  3,  6,  9,  12  =  ^,  /',  d"\  /".  Hence  from  a  theory  of  Appunn  ;  and,  of  course,  nntil  the 
the  summational  tones  we  have  8  and  9  as  reality  of  the  differentials  assumed  can  be 
partials,  while  5  =  8-3,  7=^—2,  10^12-2,  proved,  remains  a  merely  theoretical  explana- 
Ii«i2~(3  —  2),  and  so jon.    *  Therefore, *  adds  tion. 

Art.  5. — Theory  of  Beats,  Beat-Notes,  and  Combinational  Tones. 

(a)  Origin  of  Beats.  *  How  do  the  beats  of  mistuned  consonances  arise  ? '  asks 
B.  vi.  228,  and  replies  :  *  They  may  be  regarded  as  springing  from  interference  of 
new  notes,  which  arise  by  transformation,  in  the  passage  of  the  resultant  forms 
through  the  transmitting  mechanism  of  the  ear,  before  the  analysis  of  the  sen- 
sorium.* 

The  theory  of  beats  of  a  disturbed  unison  on  the  hypothesis  of  interference  is 
given  in  H.  164.  The  theory  of  differential  and  summational  tones  is  given  in 
H.  159a  and  App.  XIL  pp.  411-413.  This,  however,  extends  only  to  the  first 
differential  and  first  summational  tone.  But  H.  1586,  c,  gives  a  theory  for  the 
f  generation  of  such  tones  within  the  ear  owing  to  the  non-symmetrical  structure  of 
its  drumskin  and  the  looseness  of  the  joint  between  the  hammer  and  anvil  within 
the  drum.  And  B.  vi.  242-8,  by  means  of  some  perhaps  rather  hazardous  assump- 
tions, succeeds  in  shewing  that  the  asymmetry  of  the  drumskin  acting  upon 
the  waves  of  air  coming  to  them  would,  as  he  terms  it  in  the  above  extract, 
*  transform '  the  result  into  one  for  which  the  displacement  is  not  relatively 
infinitesimal,  but  in  which  its  higher  terms  must  be  taken  in  consideration.  Tlien 
proceeding  to  the  fourth  order  of  displacement,  he  ultimately  obtains  six  summa- 
tional and  six  differential  tones  *  produced  by  direct  transformation  of  the  prima- 
ries '  (B.  vi.  246),  so  that  he  avoids  the  introduction  of  numerous  differential  tones 
of  various  orders  (H.  200-203,  B.  vi.  241),  which  H.  seems  to  have  borrowed  fix)m 
Scheibler,  who,  although  he  did  great  things  with  the  beats  of  tuning-forks,  was 
not  a  physical  authority.  Calculation  based  on  the  introduction  of  these  entirely 
hypothetical,  because  always  inaudible,  tones  leads,  as  K.  i.  200  shews,  to  the  right 
number  of  beats ;  but,  as  he  says,  *  we  are  compelled  continually  to  assume  the 
existence  of  tones  which  have  not  only  not  been  heard  themselves,  but  which  are 

Digitized  by  V^J-OOQi€ 


SECT.  L.    EECENT  WORK  ON  BEATS  &  COMBINATIONAL  TONES.    533 

supposed  actually  to  generate  and  be  generated  by  other  likewise  infl.udible  tones/ 
"We  have  an  example  in  the  first  differential  of  a  tone  which,  when  it  does  not 
coincide  with  the  lower  beat-note,  is  appreciable  only  by  beats  with  an  auxiliary 
tone,  and  is  hence  very  faint  indeed  in  respect  to  the  generators ;  and  yet  these  are 
supposed  to  be  the  progenitors  of  others  relatively  weaker,  till  at  last  they  produce 
one  strong  enough  to  be  well  heard  (K.  i.  186).  The  diflBculty  is  surmounted  by  B., 
so  far  as  the  existence  of  the  ultimate  tone,  without  assuming  the  action  of  hypo- 
thetical intermediate  generators.  But  we  know  nothing  of  the  strength  of  these 
xdtimate  tones  as  determined  by  the  formula,  and  we  are  constrained  to  beheve 
that  what  depends  upon  the  higher  powers  of  the  displacement,  when  the  latter  is 
not  infinitesimal  in  respect  to  the  length  of  the  wave,  must  be  extremely  small, 
not  at  all  comparable  with  the  beat-notes  actually  heard,  and  hence  must  be  in- 
sufiBcient  to  explain  them.  That  is,  we  may  admit  all  the  differential  and  summa- 
tional tones  of  H.  and  B.  without  having  approached  a  satisfactory  explanation  of 
the  main  phenomenon,  the  beat-note. 

(b)  Can  Beats  generate  Tones  ?  First,  Beats  of  Intermittence.  Now,  the  obvious  IT 
hypothesis  is  that  the  beats  coming  within  the  frequency  of  musical  notes  are 
heard  as  tones.  H.  156c,  in  mentioning  this,  states  three  objections,  of  which 
P.  viii.  27  says  that  not  one  is  at  present  tenable.  They  are :  (i)  that  this  hypo- 
thesis does  not  explain  summational  but  only  differential  tones.  On  which  P. 
remarks  that  summational  tones,  which  have  been  heard  only  when  at  least  the 
second  partial  of  the  generators  was  audible,  can  be  explained  as  differential  tones  of 
the  second  order,  as  noted  supr^,  p.  532  6.  (2)  That '  under  certain  conditions  the 
combinational  tones  exist  objectively,'  which  is  against  art.  4,  p.  531 ;  and  P.  viii.  25 
especially  observes  that  the  only  experiment  which  H.  has  cited  (Pogg.  AnnaL 
vol.  xcix.  p.  539)  to  prove  the  objective  existence  of  summational  tones  by  sand 
strewed  on  a  membrane  cannot  be  critically  examined  because  the  two  generators 
are  not  specified.  (3)  That '  the  only  tones  which  the  ear  hears  correspond  to 
pendular  vibrations  of  the  air.'  P.  considers  this  to  be  disproved  by  the  inter- 
mittence  tones  obtained  by  K.,  who  rotated  a  disc  perforated  with  128  holes  before 
tuning  forks  of  different  pitches,  and  obtained  the  same  tone  of  intermittence  what- 
ever was  the  pitch  of  the  fork.  This  tone  was  accompanied  by  two  variant  tones  f 
having  pitch  numbers  equal  to  the  sum  and  difference  of  the  frequencies  of  the 
fork  and  intermittences. 

E.  i.  230,  varied  the  experiment  by  oon-  qnickly,  the  16  periods  of  the  first,  then  the 

fifructing  a  disc  *  with  three  circles,  each  with  12  of  the  second,  and  finally  the  8  of  the  third 

96  equidistant  holes,  the  diameters  of  which  circle,  passed  over  into  a  musical  tone.   Finally, 

increased  and  diminished  on  the  first  circle  16  when  the  high  tone  of  the  96  holes  on  revolv- 

iimes  from  i  to  6  mm.  (a'04  to  -24  inch),  on  ing  8  times  in  a  second  had  reached  </'  with 

the  second  12,  and  on  the  third  8  times.    On  768  d.  vib.,  the  deep  tones  c,  G,  C— answering 

blowing  through  a  tube  6  mm.  (»'24  inch)  in  to  the  numbers  of  the  periods  128,  96,  64  d. 

diameter,  and  revolving  the  disc  slowly,  the  vib. — could  be  heard  loudly  and  powerfully  at 

separate  periods  of  holes  on  each  circle  gave  the  same  tim^  as  y".' 
separate  beats.    On  revolving  continuaUy  more 

(c)  Can  Beats  generate  Tones  f  Secondly ,  Beats  of  Interference.  Now,  in 
reference  to  these  tones  of  intermittence,  E.  i.  231  remarks  that  although  they  show 
great  similarity  to  beating  combinations,  as  proving  the  possibility  of  separate 
maxima  of  intensity  passing  over  into  a  continuous  tone,  they  were  in  reality  very  « 
different  from  such  combinations,  because  in  the  case  of  beats  there  was  a  change 
of  sign,  a  maximum  of  condensation  being  followed  by  a  maximum  of  rarefaction. 

This  was  precisely  the  objection  made  by  equal  to  half  the  difference  of  the  frequencies 
liord  Bayleigh  when  Mr.  Spottiswoode  gave  of  the  primaries.'  Hien  B.  says  that  if  the 
his  account  of  Eoenig's  experiments  (Proc.  law  held  for  widely  separated  notes,  as  for  the 
Mu»,  Ass.  1878-9,  p.  128),  and  he  in  conse-  *  Fifth  (4:6),  the  note  heard  would  be  the 
quence  could  not  understand  how  beats  could  major  Third,  which  would  beat  rapidly  .  .  . 
generate  tone.  B.  {ibid,  p.  129)  raised  the  but  as  a  matter  of  fact  the  note  5  is  not  heard 
same  objection,  which  he  developed  in  B.  vi.  at  all  in  the  above  case.'  Further,  *  supposing 
223-5.  He  there  shews  that  in  the  case  of  that  in  some  unexplained  way  the  beats  whose 
two  tones  of  equal  strength,  less  than  two  speed  is  '  half  the  difference  of  the  ^equencies 
commas  from  a  unison,  *  the  resultant  dis-  of  the  primaries,  as  just  stated,  *  gave  rise  to  a 
placement '  would  produce  a  tone  *  whose  fre-  note  as  supposed  by  K.,  then  the  speed  of  that 
quency  is  the  arithmetical  mean  between  the  note  does  not  agree  with  that  required  for  E.'s 
frequencies  of  the  two  primaries,  and  having  first  [lower]  beat-note,  which  has  the  same 
oscillations  of  intensity  whose  frequency  is  speed  as  H.'s  difference-tone,'  or  the  whole  in- 
defined  by  a  pendulum  vibration  of  frequency  stead  of  half  the  difference  of  the  frequencies. 

Digitized  by  VjOOQlC 


534 


ADDITIONS  BY  THE  TRANSLATOR. 


APF.  XX. 


Now  this  objection  was  fully  realised  by  E. 
i.  232-3,  which  paper  was  before  B.  when  he 
wrote  the  passage  just  cited,  but  was  possibly 
overlooked  by  him.  K.  i.  232  says:  *If  two 
tones  of  80  and  96  d.  vib.  are  sounded  together, 
they  generate  a  tone  of  i.  (80  +  96)  =88  vibrar- 
tions  with  an  intensity  increasing  and  di- 
minishing 16  times,  and  at  each  passage  from 
one  beat  to  another  there  is  a  change  of  sign, 
so  that  the  maximum  of  compression  of  the 
first  vibration  of  the  following  beat  is  hidf 
a  vibration  behind  the  maximum  of  compres- 
sion of  the  last  vibration  of  the  preceding 
beat.'  To  meet  this  case  he  made  two  experi- 
ments. In  the  first  he  divided  a  circle  into 
176  parts,  and  in  the  five  points  i,  3,  5,  7,  9  he 
drilled  five  holes,  gradually  increasing  and  ihen 

^  diminishing  in  size.  Similarly  in  the  points 
12,  14,  16,  18,  20,  and  then  in  the  points 
23f  25.  27,  29,  and  30,  and  so  on.  *  When  such 
a  disc  was  blown  upon  through  a  pipe  with  the 
diameter  of  the  largest  opening,  in  addition  to 
the  tone  SS  and  the  very  powerful  tone  of  the 
period  16  both  of  the  tones  80  and  96  could 
be  heard,  but  they  were  very  weak,  and,  on 
account  of  the  roughness  of  the  deep  tone, 
difficult  to  observe.*  In  this  case  the  phase 
was  the  same  throughout.  To  imitate  the 
change  of  phase,  K.  i.  233  divided  each  of 
two  concentric  circles,  running  parallel  to  each 
other,  into  88  parts,  and  *  disposed  the  holes 
which  were  to  represent  the  successive  beats 
alternately  on  each.  As  88  holes  and  16 
periods  give  5^  holes  to  each  period,  E.  took 
two  periods  together,  and  pierced  on  the  first 
circle  the  divisional  points  i,  2,  3,  4,  5,  6,  and 

f  on  the  second  6,  7,  8,  9,  10,  11,  then  again  on 
the  first  12,  13,  14,  15,  16,  17,  and  on  the 
second  17,  18,  19,  20,  21,  22,  and  so  forth.' 


The  sizes  of  the  holes  were  alternately  in- 
creasing and  diminishing  to  represent  beats. 
*  When  these  circles  of  holes  were  blown  upon 
at  the  same  time  through  two  pipes  of  the 
diameter  of  the  largest  opening,  and  placed  on 
the  same  radius,  one  circle  from  above  and  the 
other  from  below,  then  at  each  revolution  of 
the  disc  there  were  created  SS  isochronous 
impulses,  varying  16  times  in  intensity,  which 
changed  sign  on  each  transition  from  one  period 
of  intensity  to  the  other.  In  this  experiment 
the  two  tones  80  and  96  were  morcf  distinct 
than  in  the  first  experiment,  where  the  circles 
of  holes  were  blown  upon  from  one  side  only.* 
On  B.'s  objections  just  quoted  (supr&,  p. 
533), E.  observes  (French  edition,  p.  143,  note) : 
*The  change  of  phase  of  the  separate  vibra- 
tions of  a  variable  amplitude,  forming  the 
beats,  does  not  cause  these  maxima  of  in- 
tensity to  be  produced  in  contrary  directions. 
Besides,  these  maxima  remain  isochronous, 
and  consequently  fulfil  the  conditions  under 
which  primary  impulses  are  combined  to  form 
sounds.  The  only  influence  which  the  change 
of  phase  in  question  exerts  on  the  disposition 
of  the  waves  consists  in  these  maxima  of  in- 
tensity not  standing  apart  by  a  whole  number 
of  complete  vibrations,  but  by  an  odd  number 
of  half-vibrations.  The  disc  of  the  siren  in 
which  the  resultant  compressions  of  all  the 
successive  vibrations  of  the  complex  sound  are 
represented  by  holes  of  a  proper  size,  and,  still 
better,  the  disc  that  has  its  rim  cut  oat 
according  to  the  curve  of  a  series  of  successive 
beats  [art.  5  (e)  below],  render  this  mechanism 
readily  apprehensible,  and  allow  of  shewing 
that,  notwithstanding  the  change  of  phase, 
the  beat-note  must  always  have  Uie  same  fre- 
quency as  the  beats.' 


(d)  Would  a  Tone  generated  by  Beats  be  louder  than  the  Primaries  ?  K.  i.  234 
then  proceeded  to  meet  Tyndall*s  objection  {On  Sound,  3rd  ed.  p.  350)  that  if  the 
resultant  tones  (as  he  calls  them)  were  formed  from  the  beats  of  the  primaries 
they  would  be  heard  when  the  primaries  were  weak,  which  is  not  the  case.  E. 
observes  that  beats  would  always  be  more  powerful  than  their  primary  tones, 
'  provided  that  equal  amplitudes  of  vibration  produced  equal  intensities  for  all 
tones,'  and  proceeds  to  shew  by  experiment  that  this  is  not  the  case,  and  that '  deep 
tones  must  have  much  larger  amplitudes  of  vibration  than  high  tones  in  order  to 
exhibit  the  same  intensity/  . 

(e)  Experiments  with  the  Wave  Siren.  Thus  the  question  was  left  till  188 1, 
when  K.  applied  his  Wave  Siren,  originally  exhibited  in  the  London  International 
Exhibition  of  1872,  already  partly  described  in  art.  2  (6),  p.  529,  to  solve  the  ques> 
tion  experimentally.     The  complete  form  (K.  v.  386)  was  of  course  applicable  to 

«|f  any  pairs  of  tones  with  ratios  expressible  by  numbers  not  exceeding  16.  But  the 
simplest  method  was  to  draw  out  the  two  harmonic  curves,  and  the  result  of  their 
combination,  as  is  done  above  (H.  30  i,  c)  on  a  very  large  scale,  and  then  reduce 
the  drawing  by  photography  to  the  required  dimensions.  Then  the  compound  curve 
t^jus  drawn  was  inverted,  so  that  the  high  parts  became  low  and  the  low  high,  cut, 
and  affixed  to  the  rim  of  the  wave  siren.  The  reason  for  inversion  in  this  case  was 
that  the  heights  on  the  curve  represented  greater  intensities,  but  on  the  siren 
would  give  less  intensities. 


K.  iii.  345  then  says :  •  When  a  disc  with . 
such  a  rim  is  rotated  before  a  slit  fixed  over  it 
in  the  direction  of  the  radius,  and  of  a  length 
at  least  equal  to  the  greatest  height  of  the  curve, 
the  slit  will  be  periodically  shortened  and  length- 
ened according  to  the  law  of  the  curve  ;  and  if 
wind  is  blown  through  the  slit,  a  motion  in 
the  air  must  be  generated  corresponding  to 
the  same  law.    And  this  motion  must  be  pre- 


cisely the  same  as  that  produced  by  the  simul- 
taneous sounding  of  two  really  simple  tones 
without  any  admixture  of  upper  partials.*  The 
beauty  of  this  arrangement  thus  consists  in  oar 
knowing  precisely  what  tones  act,  and  that 
they  are  undoubtedly  simple.  The  result  is 
thus  described :  *  The  discs  for  different  inter- 
vals, when  the  rotation  was  slow,  gave  beats, 
and  when  it  was  more  rapid,  beat-notes,  exactly 


Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


BiicT.  L.    recent:  work  on  beats  &  COMBINATIONAL  TONES.    535 


corresponding  to  those  observed  when  two 
'tuning-forks  were  sounded  together.  Thus  the 
major  Second  8  :  9  produced  the  lower  beat- 
r&ote  I ;  the  major  Seventh  8  :  is,  the  upper 
l>eat-note  i ;  the  disturbed  Twelfth  8  :  23,  the 
upper  beat-note  of  the  second  period,  which  is 


again  ^  i,  loudly  and  distinctly.  In  the  same 
way  the  ratios  8:11  and  8  :  13  gave  quite 
distinctly  and  at  the  same  time  the  upper  and 
lower  beat-notes  3  and  5  for  the  first,  and  5 
and  3  for  the  second:*  11 -8  =  3, 2  x  8-11  =  5, 
and  13-8=5,  2x8-13  =  3. 


(/)  Beats  and  Beat-Notes  heard  together.  The  preceding  experiment  shews  the 
gradual  passage  of  beats  into  tones,  the  transitional  part  being  where  both  beats 
and  tones  are  heard  together.  This  occurs  where  the  rotation  is  sufficiently  quick 
to  generate  a  tone  (see  H.  174-9,  and  especially  footnote  t  to  p.  176),  but  not  so 
fast  as  to  destroy  the  distinct  perceptions  of  beat 


To  this  I  drew  attention  in  a  footnote  to 
p,  231  of  the  1st  edition  of  this  translation, 
now  reproduced  in  a  modified  form  (suprii,  p. 
1 530^,  note) .  This  hearing  of  the  two  phenomena 
K.  i.  227  explains  by  a  theory  of  H.  (contained 
on  pp.  217-^  of  the  ist  English  ed.,  but 
omitted  in  this  2nd  English  ed.,  because  it  was 
struck  out  in  the  4th  German  ed.,  H.  having 
altered  his  opinion)  that  tones  are  heard  in 
the  cochlea  and  noises  in  other  parts  of  the 
ear.  In  the  additions  to  the  4th  Qerman  ed. 
^8upr&,  pp.  1506  to  151(2)  H.  attributes  the  hear- 
ing of  both  musical  tone  and  noise  to  the 
cochlea,  and  reserves  the  labyrinth  for  the 
sensation  of  revolution  of  the  head,  thus 
agreeing  with  Exner.  P.  viii.  29-33,  thinks 
there  are  many  reasons  why  we  should  not 
accept  the  theory  that  all  perceptions  of  noise 
are  due  to  the  cochlea.  If  so,  he  says,  *  animals 
-without  a  cochlea  would  be  deaf.  Fishes  cer- 
tainly are  mostly  dumb,  and  do  not  hear 
acutely,  as  anglers  well  know,  but  they  are 
not  deaf.*  On  examining  Exner's  paper 
(supr^,  p.  I  Sid,  note  *),  and  especially  Anna 
Tomaszewicz*s  *  Contributions  to  the  Physi- 
ology of  the  Labyrinth  of  the  Ear,'  {BeitrCtgeeur 
Physiologie  des  OhrlabyrinthSy  Medic.  In- 
augural Dissertation,  Zurich,  1877),  with  other 
phenomena,  he  comes  to  the  conclusion  that 
the  cochlea  hears  only  musical  tones  with  a 
pitch-number  not  less  than  about  16  (the 
lowest  audible  musical  tone  as  usuaUy  pro- 
duced), and  that  separate  noises  are  heard  by 
other  parts  of  the  ear — if  not  in  the  vestibule, 
then  in  the  sacculus.  He  considers  it  probable, 
as  others  have  also  thought,  that  the  function 
of  the  semicircular  canals  is  rather  to  give  a 
sensation  of  the  direction  whence  sound  comes. 
The  point  is,  however,  still  undecided. 

E.,  in  the  French  republication  of  his 
paper  (p.  137),  says:— -'At  all  events  the 
simultaneous  perception  of  separate  beats  and 
the  sound  which  results  from  their  succes- 
sion is  no  more  in  contradiction  with  the  new 


hypothesis  than  with  the  old,  for  we  can  very 
well  suppose  that,  beside  the  general  excite- 
ment of  the  basilar  membrane  due  to  each 
separate  beat,  the  particular  parts  of  this 
membrane,  whose  proper  tones  correspond  to  ^ 
the  period  of  the  impulses,  are  more  strongly 
shaken,  and  execute  lasting  vibrations  giving 
the  perception  of  sound.' 

Lord  Bayleigh,  in  his  Presidential  Address 
to  the  British  Association  meeting  at  Montreal, 
Canada,  in  Aug.  1884,  says  : — •  Every  day  we 
are  in  the  habit  of  recognising,  without  much 
difficulty,  the  quarter  from  which  a  sound 
proceeds,  but  by  what  step  we  attain  that  end 
has  not  yet  been  satisfactorily  explained.  It 
has  been  proved  that,  when  proper  precautions 
are  taken,  we  are  unable  to  distinguish  whether 
a  pure  tone  (as  from  a  vibrating  tuning-fork 
held  over  a  suitable  resonator)  comes  to  us 
from  in  front  or  from  behind.  This  is  what 
might  have  been  expected  from  an  d  priori 
point  of  view  ;  but  what  would  not  have  been 
expected  is,  that  with  almost  any  other  sort 
of  sound  the  discrimination  is  not  only  pos-  f 
sible,  but  easy  and  instinctive.  In  these  cases 
it  does  not  appear  how  the  possession  of  two 
ears  helps  us,  though  there  is  some  evidence 
that  it  does ;  and  even  when  sounds  come  to 
us  from  the  right  or  left,  the  explanation  of 
the  ready  discrimination  which  is  then  pos- 
sible with  pure  tones  is  not  so  easy  as  might 
at  first  appear.  We  should  be  inclined  to 
think  that  the  sound  was  heard  much  more 
loudly  with  the  ear  that  is  turned  towards 
than  with  the  ear  which  is  turned  from  it,  and 
that  in  this  way  the  direction  was  recognised. 
But  if  we  try  the  experiment  we  find  that— at 
any  rate  with  notes  near  the  middle  of  the 
musical  scale — the  difference  of  loudness  is  by 
no  means  so  very  great.  The  wave-lengths  of 
such  notes  are  long  enough,  in  relation  to  the 
dimensions  of  the  head,  to  forbid  the  forma- 
tion of  anything  like  a  sound-shadow  in  which  % 
the  averted  ear  might  be  sheltered.' 


ig)  Beat-Notes  and  Beat-Tones.  After  K.'s  final  experiment  (p.  532^^')  on  the 
passage  of  beats  into  tones,  we  might  perhaps  disuse  the  interim  term  '  beat>note,' 
which  implied  no  theory  as  to  its  origin,  but  only  a  statement  as  to  its  frequency, 
and  use  K.'s  term  '  beat-tone,'  implying  that  the  tone  is  generated  by  beats.  But 
just  because  *  beat-note '  does  not  imply  a  theory,  and  because  no  theory  has  been 
at  present  generally  accepted,  nor  is  sufficiently  supported  by  proofs  to  be  so,  it 
will  be  convenient  to  continue  the  use  of  the  word  *  beat-note,'  which  simply 
states  that  the  frequency  of  the  beat  is  identical  with  that  of  the  note.  At  the 
same  time  we  must  not  disuse  the  terms  '  differential  and  summational  tones,  of 
various  orders,'  because  if  they  really  exist  they  are  a  decidedly  diflFerent  pheno- 
menon from  beat -notes,  and  only  in  the  most  frequently  observed  case  coincide 
in  pitch  (but  not  in  intensity)  with  beat-notes.  P.  viii.  29,  however,  decides  to 
identify  the  two.  K.,  on  the  other  hand,  considers  the  existence  of  differential  and 
siumnational  tones  not  proved. 


Digitized  by 


Google 


535 


ADDITIONS  BY  THE  TRANSLATOR. 


APP.  XX. 


Messrs.  Preece  and  Strob,  referring  to  their 
machines  for  the  synthesis  of  vowels,  noticed 
infrd,  sect.  M.  art.  2,  p.  542^,  say  (Proc.  R. 
8.  27  Feb.  1879,  vol.  xxviii.  p.  366) :  *  The 
eurves  arrived  at  synthetically  do  .not  differ 
materially  from  those  arrived  at  analytically 
by  H.  They  principally  differ  in  the  promi- 
nence of  the  prime.  But  the  prime  can  be 
dispensed  with  altogether.  Curves  produced 
by  the  synthetic  machine,  compounded  of  the 
different  partials  without  their  prime,  shew 
that  there  exist  beats  or  resultant  sounds.  A 
vowel  sound  of  the  pitch  of  the  prime  may 
be  produced  by  certain  partials  alone,  with- 
out sounding  the  prime  at  all.  The  beat,  in 
fact,  becomes  the  prime.  This  point  is  clearly 
illustrated  by  the  automatic  phonograph,  and 


graphically  by  the  sketch  drawn  by  the  syn- 
thetic curve  machine.  In  fact,  every  two 
partials  of  numbers  indivisible  by  any  common 
multiple  [divisor?],  if  sounded  alone,  reproduce 
by  their  beats  the  prime  itself.  Thus,  the 
3rd  and  5th  partials,  or  the  2nd  and  3rd, 
<&c.,  will  result  in  the  reproduction  of  the 
prime.'  Observe  that  this  gives  the  beat-note, 
not  the  differential  tone.  The  differential 
tone  of  3  and  5  is  2,  but  the  beat-note  is 
2x3~5»i.  *In  fact,  the  figure  illustrates  not 
only  this,  but  it  shews  that  when  the  number 
of  partials  introduced  is  increased,  the  beats 
become  more  and  more  pronounced.*  Mr. 
Stroh  from  his  own  experience  considers  the 
the  beat-notes  thus  produced  to  be  generated 
in  the  same  way  as  K.  supposes. 


H  (7^)  Koenig's  explanation  of  SummatiovM  Tones.  In  the  matter  of  snimna- 
tional  tones,  P.  (see  p.  5326)  explains  them  as  differential  tones  of  the  second 
order.  K.  i.  217-8  thinks  that  they  arise  as  beat-notes  from  upper  partials.  But 
P.  viii.  24  notes  that  this  explanation  fails  when  very  high  partials  would  be 
required. 


but  the  explanation  proposed  by  M.  Preyer  is 
absolutely  inadmissible,  for  496  and  528  d.  vib., 
even  when  they  have  considerable  force,  give  32 
beats,  which  do  not  as  yet  allow  the  deep  tone 
C,  to  be  heard,  so  that  at  any  rate  such  tone 
must  be  extremely  weak.  Now  the  Octave  of 
528  (or  1056)  is  the  33rd  harmonic  of  this 
excessively  weak  sound.  But  two  primary 
sounds  of  32  and  1056  d.  vib.,  even  when 
extremely  powerful,  never  produce  a  sound  of 
1024  d.  vib.  The  second  manner  in  which  M. 
Preyer  thinks  the  sound  might  have  been  pro- 
duced is  equally  opposed  to  all  that  has  been 
directly  observed  when  two  primary  tones 
sound  together.  Thus  he  makes  the  Octave  of 
528  (i.e.  1056)  produce  with  496  d.  vib.  a  dif- 
ferential tone  of  560  d.  vib.,  and  then  makes 
this  tone  560  produce  with  the  Twelfth  of  528 
(i.e.  1584)  a  new  differential  of  1024.  Bat 
these  two  sounds  of  496  and  1056  ( «=  2  x  496  + 
64)  give  the  beat-note  64,  and  not  560 ;  and  if 
the  sound  560  really  existed  it  would  give  with 
1584  («2  X  560 +  464^ 3  X  560-96)  the  beat- 
note  96,  and  also  more  faintly  464,  but  not 
1024.* 


Thus,  to  get  the  summational  tone  64  from 
31  :  33  we  should  require  the  32nd  partial, 
which  is  not  heard.  So,  from  the  reed  tones 
496  and  528,  P.  heard  1024.  The  32nd  par- 
tials would  be  16896  and  15872,  difference 
1024.  But  such  partials  are  inaudible,  *  whereas 
every  term  of  the  acoustical  equations 

2x528-      (528-496)  =  1056-  32=1024 
3  X  528 -  (2  .  528-496)  =  1584 - 560  =  1024 

is  easily  proved.'     The  tones  mentioned  cer- 
tainly exist;   the  question  is  only,  are  they 
m  powerful  enough  to  produce  the  result  ? 

To  the  above  remarks  K.  replies  in  the 
French  edition,  p.  127,  note,  continuing  the 
passage  already  quoted  (p.  5326') :  *  M.  Preyer 
cites  in  favour  of  his  views  that  on  sounding 
together  free  reeds  of  496  and  528  d.  vib. 
■=31  '-  33fbe  heard  the  sound  1024  d.  vib.  =  64, 
and  he  thinks  that  we  cannot  assume  that  the 
reeds  had  the  32nd  partial,  16896  and  15872 
d.  vib.  If  the  sound  really  observed  was  64, 
and  not  the  Octave  of  31  or  33,  we  might  be 
really  astonished  that  the  32nd  partials  were 
snfficiently  strong  in  these  tones  to  produce  it ; 

(i)  Koenig's  theory  for  the  origin  of  Beat-Notes.  K.  i.  186  gives  the  following 
theory  for  the  origin  of  tones  from  beats.  He  says  that  '  the  beats  of  the  harmonic 
intervals,  as  well  as  of  the  unison,  should  be  deduced  directly  from  the  composition 
of  waves  of  sound,  and  we  should  assume  that  they  arise  from  the  periodically 
«-  alternating  coincidences  of  similar  maxima  of  the  generating  tones,  and  of  the 
maxima  with  opposite  signs.  The  similar  maxima  for  these  harmonic  intervals,  as 
in  the  case  of  unisons,  will  either  exactly  coincide,  or  else  there  will  be  maxima  of 
condensation  in  the  higher  tone  lying  between  two  successive  vibrations  of  the 
fundamental  tone,  slightly  preceding  one  and  slightly  following  the  other ;  but  in 
both  cases  the  effect  on  the  ear  will  be  the  same,  for  a  beat  (fluctuation)  is  no 
instantaneous  phenomenon,  but  arises  from  a  gradual  increase  and  diminution 
of  the  intensity  of  tone.*  Then  he  adds  some  drawings  of  the  compounded  vibra- 
tions of  two  tuning-forks,  one  of  which  bore  a  piece  of  smoked  glass  and  the  other 
a  style.  These  are  almost  precisely  the  same  as  the  curves  drawn  by  means  of 
Donkin's  harmonograph,  and  inserted  at  the  end  of  B.  vi.,  opposite  p.  256.  That 
is,  both  K.  and  B.,  who  are  strongly  opposed  in  opinion,  refer  to  practically 
identical  curves  in  support  of  their  own  views.  This  serves  to  shew  the  extremely 
difficult  and  delicate  nature  of  the  investigation. 

(k)  Lecture  Demonstration  of  Beat- Notes.    In  the  beat-notes  produced  by  the 
wave  siren,  K.  had  the  great  advantage  of  producing  tones  which  could  be  contmaed 

Digitized  by^OOQlC 


SECT,  u    EECENT  WORK  ON  BEATS  &  COMBINATIONAL  TONES.    537 

for  any  length  of  time,  whereas  those  from  tuning-forks  vanished  so  rapidly  that 
they  could  be  with  diflBculty  recognised.  But  this  did  not  suffice  for  lecture 
demonstrations.  Hence  K.  invented  a  machine  which  produces  beat-tones  audible 
over  a  whole  lecture  room. 

This  consists  (E.  iy.)  of  pairs  of  glass  tubes  tones  and  either  one  or  both  of  the  beat-notes 

adjusted  so  as  to  give  notes  with  definite  inter-  continnoasly,  and  load  enough  to  be  appre- 

vals  by  longitudinal  vibrations.  These  are  held  dated  by  the  whole    audience.    A  piece  of 

at  the  node  by  two  olamps  against  the  surface  paper  wrapped  round  the  node  and  bearing  the 

of  a  wheel  bearing  a  thick  cloth  tire,  which  con-  number  of  the  relative  pitch  enables  the  glass 

tinually  dips  into  a  trough  of  water,  and  thus  tubes  to  be  selected  and   changed  with  the 

rubs  the  tubes  sufficiently  to  produce  loud  greatest  rapidity. 

Art.  6. — Infltience  of  difference  of  Phase  on  Quality  of  Tone. 
H.  p.  126a  finds  that  dmerence  of  phase  has  no  effect  on  quality  of  tone.  But, 
on  p.  127c,  H.  points  out  'an  apparent  exception,*  on  which  E.  v.  376  remarks 
that  if  quality  depends  on  the  relative  intensity  of  the  harmonic  upper  partials,  % 
and  this  relative  intensity  is  really  altered  by  difference  of  phase,  the  influence  of 
this  difference  is  '  actual,  and  not  merely  apparent.'  Then  observing  on  the 
difficulties  attendant  on  H.'s  rule  for  finding  the  differences  of  phase  (supr&, 
p.  124c),  he  proceeds  to  describe  his  new  experiments  with  the  wave  siren  (for 
which  reason  they  are  mentioned  in  this  place),  which  certainly  admit  of  very 
much  more  precision.  They  were  conducted  thus :  £.  compounded  harmonic 
curves  of  various  pitches,  and  with  various  assumptions  of  amplitudes,  under  four 
varieties  of  phase :  (i)  the  beginning  of  all  the  waves  coinciding ;  (2)  the  first 
quarter,  (3)  the  halves,  and  (4)  the  third  quarter  of  each  wave  coinciding ;  briefly 
said  to  have  a  difference  of  phase  of  o,  -j,  ^,  |.  These  were  reduced  by  photo- 
graphy, inverted,  and  placed  on  the  rim  of  the  disc  of  a  wave  siren,  and  then 
made  to  speak.  He  gives  the  remarkable  curves  which  resulted  in  a  few  cases, 
and  instructions  for  repeating  the  experiments.  The  following  are  his  conclu- 
sions (K.  V.  391) : — 

*  The  composition  of  a  number  of  harmonic  tones,  including  both  the  evenly 
Bnd  unevenly  numbered  partials,  generates  in  all  cases,  quite  independently  of  the  If 
relative  intensity  of  these  tones,  the  strongest  and  acutest  quality  tone  for  the 
\  difference  of  phase,  and  the  weakest  and  softest  for  |  difference  of  phase,  while 
the  difference  o  and  ^  lie  between  the  others,  both  as  regards  intensity  and 
acuteness. 

'  When  unevenly  numbered  partials  only  are  compounded,  the  differences  of 
phase  \  and  |  give  the  same  quality  of  tone,  as  do  also  the  differences  o  and  ^ ; 
but  the  former  is  stronger  and  acuter  than  the  latter. 

'  Hence,  although  the  quahty  of  tone  principally  depends  on  the  number  and 
relative  intensity  of  the  harmonic  tones  compounded,  the  influence  of  difference  of 
tone  is  not  by  any  means  so  insignificant  as  to  be  entirely  negligible.  We  may 
say,  in  general  terms,  that  the  differences  in  the  number  and  relative  intensity  of 
the  haisnonic  tones  compounded  produces  those  differences  in  the  quality  of  tone 
which  are  remarked  in  musical  instruments  of  different  famihes,  or  in  the  human 
voice  uttering  different  vowels.  But  the  alteration  of  phase  between  these  har- 
monic tones  can  excite  at  least  such  differences  of  quality  of  tone  as  are  observed 
in  musical  instruments  of  the  same  fojnily,  or  in  different  voices  singing  the  same  If 
vowel.* 

Of  course,  as  K.  v.  392  observes,  the  complete  wave  siren  figured  on  K.  v.  386 
is  applicable  to  numerous  other  investigations. 

Art.  7, — Influence  of  Combinational  Tones  on  the  consonance  of  Simple  Tones » 
This  is  a  brief  notice  of  P.  viii.  HI.  It  would  appear  from  H.'s  theory  of  con- 
sonance (see  especially  supra,  pp.  2ood  and  2056)  that,  if  there  were  no  upper 
partial  or  combinational  tones,  dissonance  and  consonance  could  not  be  distin- 
guished— in  the  Thirds  for  example.  P.'s  experiments  rendered  this  doubtful.  He 
had  a  series  of  1 1  forks  made,  very  accurately  tuned  to — 

Vib.    1000    iioo    1200    1300    1400    1500    1600    1700    1800    1900    2000 
Cents         165       151     138       129      119       112       105       99        93        89 
Sums       o     165       316      454     583       702     814      919     1018    mi    1200 

where  the  upper  line  gives  the  numbers  of  vibrations,  the  second  the  cents  in  the 

Digitized  by  VjOOQlC 


538  ADDITIONS  BY  THE  TEANSLATOB.  aPf.xx. 

intervals  between  two  snccessiye  forks,  and  the  bottom  the  sums  of  those  cents, 
or  the  cents  in  the  intervals  between  any  fork  and  the  lowest,  from  which  the 
cents  in  the  intervals  between  any  two  forks  can  be  immediately  deduced  by  sub- 
traction ;  and  by  a  reference  to  the  table  in  Sect.  D.  the  names  of  such  intervals  can 
be  found.  P.  selected  the  difference  of  loo  vib.  because  it  was  small  enough  to  allow 
of  a  sensation  of  roughness  when  two  successive  forks  were  sounded  together. 
And  he  selected  the  pitches  looo  and  2000  because  they  precluded  hearing  upper 
partials,  while  the  frequency  1000  to  2000  not  being  sufficient  to  have  any  dOTect 
on  distinguishing  consonance,  the  absence  of  power  to  distinguish  it  could  not  be 
ascribed  to  the  high  pitch. 

Both  practised  and  onpractised  ears  imme-  pair  of  sounds,  it  loses  the  disagreeable  effect 

diately  recognised  on  them  that  successive  forks  of  dissonance.* 

were  dissonant  to  each  other;  this  was  due  The  Octave  and  Fifth  were  generally  re- 

to  the  small  difference  of  100  vib.    Almost  all  cognised  with  certainty,  probably  from  long 

other  intervals  of  these  x  I  forks,  when  the  forks  practice.    This  appears    to  be  an  excellent 

■r  were  not  too  loud,  were  frequently  considered  proof  of  H.'s  theory.    And  the  less  care  there 

'  consonant,  especially  by  musicians— such  as  was  taken  to  exclude  upper  partials  and  com- 

10  :  13,  iz  :  13,  12  :  19,  17  :  20,  <&c.    Also  the  binational  tones,  the  more  unpleasant  became 

ratios  expressible  by  small  numbers  (except  the  dissonance,  and  the  easier  it  was  for  the 

8  :  9  and  9  :  10),  namely,  5  :  7,  5  :  9,  6  :  7,  ear  alone  to  determine  the  interval  immedi- 

7  '  8, 7  :  9, 7  :  10,  often  passed  as  consonances;  ately.    But  this  is  not  all.    H.*s  theory  that 

and  though  the  i  :  2,  2  :  3,  3  :  4,  3  :  5,  4  :  5,  dissonances  should  be  recognised  only  by  beats 

5:6,  5:8  were   genenJly   preferred,    some  of  the  partials  or  combinational  tones  implies 

observers    found    6:7,   15  :  19,   11  :  I3t   <&o*  that,  if  these  were  too  far  distant  in  pitch  to 

more  harmonious  than  the  pure  Thirds  and  produce  beats,  there  would  be  no  roughness. 

Sixths,  4  :  5}  5  :  6,  5  :  8,  3  :  5,  especially  than  and  hence  no  beats.    This  did  not  prove  to  be 

the    minor    Sixth    5  :  8.     The    listener    was  the  case.    The  pair  1400  :  1600  vib.  formed  a 

always  kept  in  ignorance  of  the  numerical  dissonance,  although  all  partials  and  combi- 

ratios,  and  only  one  person  was  tried  at  a  national  tones  differed  by    200.    The    ratio 

time.    The  sum  of  the  different  judgments  8  :  9  was  universally  called  a  cutting  disso- 

was  therefore :  nance,  even  in  the  4  times  and  8  times  accented 

*  After  aU  upper  partials  and  combinational  Octave, 
tones  have  been  eliminated  from  a  dissonant 

^  The  explanation  of  the  above  phenomena  seemed  to  require  a  remodelling  of 
H.'s  theory,  and  P.*s  conclusions  are  stated  thus  (P.  viii.  58) : — 

'  (i)  The  larger  the  least  two  numbers  required  to  express  the  ratio  between  two 
tones,  the  greater  the  nun^r  of  combinational  tones,  which  always  form  an 
arithmetical  series,  and  arise,  whether  upper  partials  be  present  or  not  (H.  155c). 

'  (2)  The  greater  the  number  of  simple  tones  which  affect  the  ear  simultaneously, 
the  less  distinct  is  each  single  tone. 

'  (3)  The  more  coincidences  there  are  between  the  tones  which  might  be  and 
are  generated  by  any  interval,  the  more  pleasing  is  the  sensation  ;  and  the  fewer 
the  coincidences  the  more  confusing,  and  hence  unpleasant,  the  impression.' 

And  as  these  conclusions  hold  for  tones  which,  on  account  of  their  own  dis- 
tance from  eadi  other  and  the  distance  of  their  partials  and  combinational  tones, 
cannot  generate  sensible  beats,  P.  considers  that  this  is  both  a  formal  and  an 
actual  extension  of  H.'s  theory  of  consonance.  But  if,  with  E.,  we  consider 
these  differential  tones  absolutely  insensible,  it  would  be  difficult  to  see  how 
they  would  affect  the  result,  and  the  facts  noted  would  still  require  explanation. 
V  The  whole  subject  of  combinational  tones  and  beats  evidently  requires  much  more 
examination. 

SECTION  M. 

ANALTBIS  AKD  SYNTHESIS  OF  VOWEL  SOUMBS. 

(See  Notes,  pp.  75,  zz8,  124.) 

Art.  Art. 

I.  Analysis  of  Vowel  Sounds  by  means  of  the      2.  Synthetical  Production  of  Yowol  Sounds, 
Phonograph,  p.  538.  p.  542. 

Art.  I. — Analysis  of  Vowel  Sounds  by  means  of  the  Phonograph.  The  follow- 
ing is  a  brief  account  of  a  paper  by  Prof.  Fleeming  Jenkin,  F.R.SS.  L.  and  E., 
and  Mr.  J.  A.  Ewing,  B.Sc,  F.R.S.E.,  On  the  Harmonic  Analysis  of  certain 
Vowel  Sounds,  *  Transactions  of  the  Royal  Society  of  Edinburgh,'  vol.  xxviii. 


Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


PECT.  M.    ANALYSIS  AND  SYNTHESIS  OF  VOWEL   SOUNDS. 


539 


pp.  745-777,  plates  34-40,  communicated  June  3  and  July  i,  1878,  and  published 
'With  additions  to  July  19,  1878— that  is,  subsequently  to  the  appearance  of  the  4th 
German  ed.  of  this  work. 

Messrs.  Jenkin  and  Ewing  make  use  of  a  variety  of  Mr.  T.  A.  Edison's  phono- 
graph which,  by  means  of  a  style  affixed  to  a  vibrating  disc  against  which  words 
are  spoken  or  sung,  impresses  the  amplitude  of  vibration  at  any  time  on  a  piece 
of  tinfoil  passed  beneath  it  by  machinery.  On  repassing  the  style  over  these 
indentations  the  vibrations  are  recommunicated  to  the  disc,  and  the  sounds  re- 
produced sufficiently,  on  the  form  of  the  instrument  used  by  these  gentlemen,  for 
listeners  to  understand  sentences  impressed  during  their  absence.  Then  the 
indented  foil  was  passed  under  another  style  in  communication  with  a  system  of 
delicate  levers,  ending  in  one  of  Sir  W.  Thompson's  electrical  squirting  recorder 
tubes,  which  magnified  the  depth  of  the  indentations  400  times,  and  squirted  their 
form,  without  friction,  on  to  a  telegraph-paper  band  wound  round  a  cylinder  re- 
volving at  such  a  speed  as  to  magnKy  the  length  of  the  indentations  7  times. 
Perfect  records  of  the  vibrations  registered  by  the  phonograph  were  thus  obtained,  If 
of  sufficient  size  to  be  measured.  The  amplitudes  of  the  compound  vibrations  of 
the  curves  were  measured  to  the  200th  part  of  an  inch  ('005  inch).  Then,  as  the 
apparatus  could  not  properly  determine  high  partials,  the  curves  were  assumed  to 
be  compounded  of  six  partials,  and  the  ordinates  or  amplitudes  had  to  be  deter- 
mined by  Fourier's  formula — 

y=-4o+'4i  sin  aj-h-ij  sin  2a;-f-  .  .  .  +-4,sinna;-f  .  .  • 
+-B,  C0QX+B2  cos  2X+  .  .  .  +B^  cos  nx-jr  .  .  . 

The  period  was  taken  as  the  length  between  two  minima  of  ordinates,  and 
divided  into  1 2  equal  parts  for  successive  values  of  a?,  and  then  the  corresponding 
values  of  y  were  measured.  The  1 2  resulting  simultaneous  equations,  giving  the 
values  Aq  to  A^  and  By  to  -B5,  were  then  solved  by  Professor  Tait's  formulaB  (given 
in  the  paper),  and  thus  the  amplitudes  of  the  six  partials  for  any  length  of  the 
ordinate  were  determined.    The  Authors  say :— •  IF 

'  The  experiments  were  chiefly  directed  to  the  two  sounds  o  and  u  (the  vowels  in  o^  / 
and  food).  Several  different  voices  were  employed.  Voice  No.  i  was  a  powerful  bari- 
tone with  a  considerable  range  and  good  musical  training.  No.  2  was  a  high  set  and 
somewhat  harsh  voice  of  limited  range  and  without  musical  training.  No.  3  was  a  rich 
and  well-trained  bass  voice  of  a  man  of  eighty.  Nos.  4  and  5  were  somewhat  alike,  being 
voices  of  moderate  range  and  power  and  with  some  musical  training.  No.  6  was  a 
powerful  bass.  Generally  the  vowels  were  sung  in  tune  with  notes  given  by  a  piano,* 
the  pitch  of  which  was  supposed  to  be  c^  256,  but  was  probably  much  higher. 

.  Photo-lithographs  of  the  records  of  the  vibratory  curves  are  given  in  the  paper, 
and  ingeniously  arranged  tables  are  added  shewing  the  maximum  amplitudes  of 
the  partials  for  each  pitch  of  the  prime.  Of  these,  the  following  is  Table  VU.  p.  761 
slightly  re-arranged,  with  the  names  of  the  upper  partials  inserted : — 


Vowel  Sound  0  {'oh'). 


Voice 

Pitches  and  AMPLmrDBB  of  thb  First  Sec  Pabtiam 

No. 

I. 

II. 

III. 

IV.          1            V. 

VI. 

/'« 

rz 

C"« 

rt 

a"'t 

c-'jf 

2 

44 

32 

6 

0 

4 

2 

/' 

/" 

&" 

r 

a'" 

d'" 

I 

121 

71 

7 

I 

5 

4 

5 

53 

19 

6 

2 

3 

I 

V" 

c' 

e" 

h" 

•  e'" 

rt 

I 

105 

69 

7 

3 

2 

3 

2 

51 

30 

5 

2 

I 

I 

3 

53 

18 

3 

I 

2 

I 

4 

55 

34 

7 

2 

2 

0 

5 

52 

53 

5 

6 

5 

2 

Digitized  by 


Google 


S40 


ADDITIONS  BY  THE  TEANSLATOB. 
VowBL  BoxmD  S  (*  OB ')—eontinwd. 


APP.  XX. 


VOICK 

PrrCHBB  AND  AlfPUTUDBB  OF  THJt  PIR8T  SlX  PaBTIALS                      | 

No. 

I. 

II. 

in. 

IV. 

V. 

VL 

J 

d' 

d" 

a" 

d'" 

/'"« 

a"' 

"J 

76 

5 

I 

3 

2 

66 

40 

4 

0 

3 

2 

27 

42 

6 

4 

2 

I 

tf 

c" 

1^' 

c"' 

c"' 

iT' 

no 

160 

15 

10 

10 

7 

37 

30 

I 

4 

I 

I 

54 

25 

0 

3 

2 

I 

1 

47 

41 

5 

3 

3 

2 

b 

1/ 

rz 

6" 

d'"« 

/"'« 

70 

126 

15 

14 

6 

I 

45 

66 

7 

4 

6 

2 

36 

31 

2 

4 

I 

0 

45 

i^ 

4 

2 

i 

0 

47 

61 

2 

14 

2 

6b 

I/b 

r 

y'\> 

d'" 

f" 

75 

185 

li 

8 

II 

I 

49 

104 

6 

4 

2 

s 

82 

5 

7 

I 

2 

70 

13 

7 

I 

3 

a 

a' 

e" 

a" 

c'"« 

e"' 

125 

190 

25 

22 

5 

2 

3a 

5! 

6 

8 

6 

2 

1 

40 

36 

4 

4 

3 

2 

16 

§i 

4 

4 

I 

I 

40 

68 

10 

8 

3 

0 

^ 

g^ 

d" 

(7" 

6" 

d"' 

103 

27 

6 

2 

2 

23 

51 

14 

3 

2 

2 

46 

29 

2 

2 

2 

I 

33 

44 

7 

2 

I 

2 

32 

50 

3 

6 

I 

2 

/» 

f\ 

c"« 

r« 

a"J 

c"'« 

18 

58 

15 

3 

I 

0 

28 

62 

10 

2 

3 

2 

/ 

/' 

c^ 

r 

a" 

c^ 

55 

140 

45 

4 

8 

I 

25 

37 

11 

3 

4 

2 

IT 

70 

109 

58 

13 

12 

5 

6 

«' 

y 

c" 

0 

6" 

72 

131 

73 

7 

10 

4 

40 

67 

35 

5 

5 

2 

25 

12 

21 

6 

6 

0 

41 

88 

64 

13 

5 

2 

d 

(f 

0' 

d" 

/"» 

a" 

44 

134 

82 

16 

22 

10 

27 

61 

38 

19 

4 

2 

20 

46 

21 

3 

4 

I 

33 

72 

56 

5 

7 

2 

e 

c' 

1^ 

c" 

e" 

iT 

I 

18 

•  95 

61 

33 

3 

0 

3 

19 

48 

33 

18 

2 

4 

Digitized  by  VjOOQIC 


3ECT.  M.  ANALYSIS  AND  SYNTHESIS  OP  VOWEL  SOUNDS. 


541 


VowH.  Souin)  S  (*  OH ') — eontimied. 


Voicx 

Pitches  akd  Akplztitdis  of  thb  Fibst  Six  Fabtialb 

No. 

I. 

11. 

IIL 

IV. 

V. 

VI. 

B 

5 

>"! 

y 

d"« 

/"« 

I 

as 

«5 

28 

31 

6 

5 

3 

21 

46 

29 

28 

10 

0 

4 

12 

34 

23 

10 

4 

I 

5 

6 

38 

23 

25 

6 

3 

J5b 

6b 

/' 

l/b 

d" 

/" 

I 

^2 

58 

61 

^I 

II 

0 

3 

28 

41 

25 

36 

9 

0 

5 

i8 

26 

15 

»S 

2 

2 

6 

l8 

22 

32 

75 

9 

2 

.1 

a 

«' 

a' 

c"« 

c" 

I 

15 

15 

18 

29 

6 

3 

3 

26 

^i 

35 

39 

18 

2 

5 

15 

8 

22 

21 

4 

0 

^ 

9 

46 

44 

80 

12 

4 

G 

0^ 

d' 

g" 

«/ 

d" 

I 

13 

0 

15 

40 

8 

4 

6 

34 

30 

8 

45 

9 

7 

J'^ 

/ 

c/ 

r 

a' 

c" 

6 

22 

10 

IS 

8 

34 

I 

The  following  is  a  table  of  the  results  for  H  for  voices  i  and  5  only,  where,  for 
brevity,  I  give  only  the  pitches  of  the  primes,  for  the  pitches  of  the  partials  are 
^ven  in  the  preceding  table,  and  the  numbering  of  the  partials  is  sufficient  to 
shew  the  great  peculiarity  of  the  jump  from  one  reinforced  partial  to  two,  the  H 
second  being  then  by  far  the  most  prominent,  and  the  different  pitches  at  which 
different  voices  make  the  change.  Voice  5  could  not  get  out  a  clear  H  at  the  pitch  a. 
To  these  are  added  the  results  obtained  from  voice  5  for  the  vowels  &^  (*  awe ')  and 
a  ('  ah  ')• 


Vowel  fl  ('oo').  Voick  5 

Vowel  «  (*oo  ');  Voice  x 

Pitch 

Amplitudtjs  OF  Partlulb 

Pitch 

Amplttudkh  of  Pahtials 

OF  PRIMK 

OF  FllIHK 

I. 
136 

n. 

III. 

IV. 

V. 

VI. 

I. 

II. 

III. 

IV. 

V. 

VI. 

d 

6 

2 

4 

3 

I 

d' 

94 

7 

0 

2 

3 

2 

d 

85 

3 

0 

3 

2 

2 

b    , 

287 

26 

12 

3 

8 

0 

bb 

250 

8' 

II 

1 

I 

2 

bb 

22 

189 

12 

38 

2 

8 

a 

13 

120 

12 

12 

4 

0 

g 

38 

128 

9 

4 

10 

II 

9 

22 

136 

6 

16 

3 

I 

f 

23 

13s 

5 

10 

3 

5 

f 

21 

108 

7 

13 

6 

2 

e 

34 

148 

18 

7 

6 

3 

e 

27 

127 

12 

14 

2 

2 

d 

33 

107 

14 

4 

3 

0 

c 

31 

74 

41 

4 

6 

3 

B 

18 

50 

28 

3 

5 

I 

VOWKL  SP  (*  AWK  '),  VOICK  5 

VOWKL  a  (•  ah  •),  VOICF.  S 

d' 

9 

22 

14 

3 

2 

2 

d 

20 

48 

58 

i 

10 

0 

b 

20 

SI 

S6 

3 

2 

a 

41 

48 

48 

3 

6 

2 

a 

37 

62 

46 

20 

4 

3 

9 

24 

44 

32 

15 

3 

0 

9 

23 

35 

24 

25 

6 

0 

f 

14 

18 

14 

23 

2 

0 

f 

24 

29 

12 

24 

I 

I 

e 

23 

39 

32 

40 

6 

2 

e 

12 

23 

II 

15 

9 

3 

d 

19 

26 

20 

30 

7 

3 

Digitized  by  V^OOQ IC 


54a  ADDITIONS  BY  THE  TRANSLATOB.  app.  xx. 

On  the  first  table  the  authors  remark : — 

'  At  the  pitches  ordinarily  used  in  speech,  the  vowel  o  consists  almost  wholly  of  the 
two  constituents — a  prime  and  its  Octave — the  ratio  of  whose  amplitudes  may  vary 
widely.  But  when  the  range  is  extended  so  as  to  reach  lower  pitches,  higher  purtials 
successively  appear  in  such  a  way  as  to  allow  the  highest  strongly  reinforced  partial  to 
remain  in  the  neighbourhood  of  &'b.  .  •  .  Generally  we  may  say,  .  .  .  that  there  is  a 
wide  range  of  reinforcement,  extending  over  about  two  Octaves  (from /or  g  to/'O*  within 
which  all  tones  are  more  or  less  strongly  reinforced,  and  that  there  is  a  specially  strong 
reinforcement  at  the  pitch  h'b.' 

But  this  last  did  not  appear  for  the  artificial  6*a  produced  by  Prof.  Gram 
Brown's  instrument  (described  in  the  paper),  and  recognised  by  the  ear  as  o's. 
They  also  draw  attention  to  the  sudden  alteration  of  amplitude  of  the  4th  partial 
with  voice  6,  and  also  of  the  sth  partial,  for  pitches  B\},  A,  O,  a,a  compared  with 
F ;  and  to  siioilar  sudden  alterations  in  the  3rd  and  also  4th  partial  with  voices 

ir  1, 3. 5- 

After  discussing  the  results  condensed  in  these  tables,  the  authors  review  former 
vowel  theories  and  give  their  own  conclusions,  of  which  the  following  may  be 
noted ;  but  the  whole  paper  requires  careful  examination,  as  a  most  original  and 
laborious  study  of  a  very  difficult  subject, 

*  In  distinguishing  vowels  the  ear  is  guided  by  two  factors,  one  depending  on  the  har- 
mony or  group  of  relative  partiaJs,  and  the  other  on  the  absolute  pitch  of  the  reinforced 
constituents.  It  seems  not  a  little  singular  that  the  ear  should  attach  so  distinct  a  unity 
to  sounds  made  up  of  such  very  various  groups  of  constituents  as  we  have  obtained  from 
different  voices  and  at  different  pitches,  so  as  to  recognise  cdl  these  sounds  as  some  one 
particular  vowel.  We  are  forced  to  the  conclusion  • . .  that  the  ear  recognises  the  kind  of 
oral  camty  by  which  the  reinforcement  is  produced. . . .  The  vowel-producing  resonance 
cavities  are  clearly  distinguished  in  virtue  of  two  properties — ^first,  the  absolute  pitch  at 
which  they  produce  a  maximum  reinforcement ;  and,  second,  the  area  of  pitch  over  which 
reinforcement  acts.  The  latter  property,  when  it  is  extensive,  is  very  probably  due  to 
the  existence  of  subordinate  j^roper  tones  not  far  from  each  other  in  pitch.  .  .  .  W'e 
m  should  .  .  .  describe  the  u  cavity  as  an  adjustable  cavity,  with  a  very  limited  range  of 
resonance,  whose  efiect  is  to  reinforce  strongly  only  one  partial  above  the  pitch  of  a.  .  .  . 
If  we  assume  that  the  o  cavity  is  absolutely  constant,  we  must  describe  it  as  a  cavity  re- 
inforcing tones  throughout  nearly  two  Octaves,  or  from  g  to  f^\  .  .  .  We  are  disposed  to 
regard  it  as  more  probable,  that  in  human  voices  the  0  cavity  is  slightly  tuned  or  modi- 
fied according  to  the  pitch  on  which  the  vowel  is  simg ;  .  .  .  the  genuine  character  of  0  is 
given  by  a  cavity  reinforcing  tones  over  rather  more  than  one  Octave,  with  an  upper 
proper  tone  never  far  from  6' b.  ...  It  is  very  satisfactory  to  find  that  the  5*8  given  by 
the  human  voices  which  we  have  experimented  with  are  marked  by  the  strong  resonance 
on  &'b  which  Helmholtz  has  noticed  by  quite  different  methods  of  observation.  It  (ends 
to  shew  that  our  0  was  essentially  the  same  vowel  sound  as  his,  and  to  give  us  confidence 
in  the  mode  of  experiment  we  have  adopted.'     {Ibid,  pp.  772-775.) 

Art.  2. — Synthetical  Production  of  Vowel  Sounds,  A  most  ingenious  method  of 
producing  artificial  vowels  was  invented,  and  is  explained  by  Messrs.  W.  H.  Preece 
and  A.  Stroh  in  their  paper  entitled  Studies  in  Acoustics  :  On  the  Synthetic  Ex- 
amination of  Vowel  SouTiids,  *  Proceedings  of  the  Eoyal  Society,'  Feb.  27,  1879,  ^^1. 
^  xxviii.  No.  193,  pp.  358-67.  Mr.  Stroh,  to  whom  all  the  machinery  is  due,  was 
kind  enough,  on  May  29, 1884,  for  the  purposes  of  this  Appendix,  t6  shew  me  the 
machines  in  action,  and  to  reproduce  the  results  many  times  over  in  order  that  I 
should  be  able  to  judge  of  them.  Essentially  there  are  four  machines.  First,  one 
to  produce  the  curve  resulting  from  compounding  8  harmonic  curves,  representing 
partials,  with  maximum  amplitudes  decreasing  inversely  as  the  number  of  the 
partial  increased,  but  with  arrangements  for  altering  the  amplitudes  and  phases 
of  composition.  The  resulting  figures  are  extremely  beautiful.  Secondly,  a 
machine  for  cutting  the  curve  thus  produced,  but  on  a  reduced  scale,  on  the  edge  of 
a  brass  disc,  so  that  30  periods  were  included  in  one  circumference  of  this  disc,  the 
curves  being  automatically  transferred  from  the  first  machine.  Third,  a  machine 
by  which  an  axis  on  which  8  of  the  discs  thus  cut  were  placed,  representing  8  par- 
tials. These  discs  by  springs  could  be  brought  into  action  in  any  combinations,  and 
could  convey  the  resulting  vibration  to  a  style  working  against  a  sensitive  disc  hke 
that  of  the  telephone.  The  sensitive  disc  on  vibrating  produced  the  corresponding 
sound  audibly.    Not  being  satisfied  with  these  results,  Mr.  Stroh  took  the  combina- 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


SECT.M.    ANALYSIS  AND  SYNTHESIS  OF  VOWEL  SOUNDS.  543 

tions  and  amplitudes  which  these  experiments  shewed  were  Hkely  to  sncceed  best, 
made  the  corresponding  compound  curves  by  the  synthetic  machine,  cut  them  by 
the  second  machine,  mounted  them  as  in  the  third,  and  then  in  a  fourth  or  vowel 
machine  conducted  the  vibrations  from  each  compound  curve  to  a  disc,  which  spoke 
them.  The  details  and  drawings  of  the  first  and  fourth  machine,  the  speaking 
disc,  and  various  compound  curves  are  given  in  the  paper.  The  curves  are  also 
compared  with  those  resulting  from  my  table  of  Prof.  Helmholtz's  results  (supdk.,  p. 
1 24c,  dy  footnote) ,  which  had  also  appeared  in  the  i  st  edition  of  this  translation,  p.  1 8 1 . 
The  table  of  the  intensities  of  the  partials  given  in  the  paper  (on  its  p.  365) — though 
I  am  not  quite  sure  that  they  agreed  with  those  I  heard — ore  as  follows,  the  pitch  of 
the  prime  being  B\}  : — 

8      16 


Vowels    I 

2      3 

4       5 

6 

U       ff 

mf   pp 

0       mf 

f    mf 

P 

A        p 

p      p 

mf   mf 

P 

E      mf 

mf 

ff 

I       mf 

P 

P 

1> 

mf 

The  effect  of  these  vowels  on  my  ear  was  not  like  that  of  human  vowels,  hence 
I  found  it  extremely  difficult  to  place  them  anywhere  in  the  human  vowel 
scale.    Boughly,  I  felt  that — 

U  was  a  sort  of  00,  tending  towards  oh  t 

0  was  more  like  the  word  awe  than  oh ! 

A  was  a  very  high  ah,  tending  to  the  long  sound  of  English  a  in  fat, 

E  was  very  imperfect,  and  had  the  effect  of  a  hollow  low  French  i  mixed* 

with  EngHsh  u  in  but. 
J,  was  the  worst  vowel.    It  had  none  of  the  character  of  ai  in  air,  but  was 

far  from  ee.    The  sound  *  tootled.' 

When  taken  in  rapid  succession,  the  ear  at  once  recognised  that  these  sounds 
were  meant  for  00,  oh,  ah,  and  perhaps  ay,  ee ;  but  on  prolonging  the  sound  of 
any  one,  the  character  of  the  vowel  became  lost,  as  indeed  is  frequently  the  case 
in  singing.  Curious  effects  resulted  from  raising  and  lowering  the  pitch.  The  O  if 
flattened  became  a  very  decent  00  (in  boot),  and  the  A  flattened  almost  a  good  oh. 
The  effect  of  taking  all  an  Octave  higher  was  not  so  successful. 

The  synthesis  of  Prof.  Helmholtz  and  that  of  Messrs.  Preece  and  Stroh, 
together  with  the  analysis  of  Messrs.  Fleeming  Jenkin  and  Ewing,  in  art.  i,  prove 
distinctly  that  difference  in  the  quaUty  of  tone,  taking  only  harmonic  partials,  is 
the  foundation  of  vowels,  and  also  that  difference  of  phase  has,  so  far  as  they  could 
observe,  no  effect  on  the  ear.  (But  see  supr^,  App.  XX.  Sect.  L,  art.  vi.  p.  536.) 
Both,  however,  also  prove  that  there  is  much  more  yet  to  be  learned  before  we 
can  satisfactorily  imitate  spoken  vowels.  Each  of  these  methods  of  synthesis 
necessarily  relates  to  sung  vowels,  which  are  quite  distinct  from  spoken  vowels,  and 
indeed  never  satisfactorily  imitate  them.  It  appears  to  me  that  the  mode  of  vibra- 
tion of  the  vocal  chords  is  a  most  important  element  of  vowel  character,  and  that 
the  resulting  effect  is  modified  by  the  resonance  in  the  ventricles  of  Morgagni,  in 
the  cartilagenous  larynx  more  or  less  covered  by  the  epiglottis  (acting,  possibly, 
like  the  cup  mouthpiece  of  brass  instruments,  see  supr^,  p.  gSd,  note),  in  the  ^ 
pharynx,  and  between  the  pillars  of  the  velum,  before  it  reaches  the  larger  re- 
sonance cavities  of  nose  and  mouth,  with  which  we  are  almost  solely  able  to  deal. 
By  the  original  mode  of  vibration  of  the  vocal  chords  for  spoken  vowels  many 
inharmonic  proper  tones  are  probably  produced,  which  are  overcome  in  singing, 
and  this  is  possibly  one  of  the  many  differences  between  speaking  and  singing. 
Also,  we  should  bear  in  mind  that  each  speaker  has  his  personal  quality  of  '  voice  * 
(that  is,  mainly,  of  vowel  sound),  by  which  he  would  be  recognised  in  the  dark,  and 
that  in  each  individual  the  feeling  of  the  moment  varies  the  pitch  and  the  charac- 
teristic quality  of  his  vowels  ;  so  that  there  are  really  millions  of  different  qualities 
of  tone  all  recognised  generically  as  the  same  vowel.  And  yet  in  the  artificial  vowels 
just  considered  I  could  not  recognise  any  exact  form  of  human  vowel  with  which 
I  was  acquainted,  although  I  have  made  speech  sounds  an  especial  study  for  more 
than  forty  years.  We  have  an  analogy  in  the  multiform  presentment  of  the 
human  countenance,  which  is  nevertheless  unhesitatingly  recognised  as  distinct 
from  that  of  the  anthropoid  ape. 

Digitized  by  VjOOQIC 


544 


ADDITIONS  BY  THE  TEANSLATOB. 


APP.  XX 


SECTION  N. 

MIBCELLANBOtTS  NOTES* 

(See  pp.  78,  179.) 


No. 

X. 

2. 


CompaBs  of  the  Hainan  Voice,  p.  544. 
Harmonics  and  Partials  of    a  Pianoforte 
String,  struck  at  one-eighth  of  its  length, 

P-  545. 
History  of  Mean  Tone  Temperament,  p.  546. 
History  of  Equal  Temperament,  p.  548. 
-Prof.  Mayer's  Analysis  of  Compound  Sounds 

and  Harmonic  Curves,  p.  549. 


No. 

6. 

7- 
8. 


Presumed  different  characters  of  Keys,  both 

Major  and  Minor,  p.  550. 
Dr.  W.  H.  Stone's  Restoration  of  i6-fbot  C, 

to  the  Orchestra,  p.  552. 
On  the  Action  of  Reeds,  from  notes  by  Mr. 

Hermann  Smith,  p.  553. 
Postscript,  p.  555. 


Compass  of  the  Suman  Voice, 


Instruments  can  be  tuned  or  manufactured 
at  almost  any  required  pitch.  The  human 
voice  is  bom,  not  manufactured.  Although  by 
skilful  training  its  compass  can  generally  be 
somewhat  extended,  both  upwards  and  down- 
wards, yet  it  must  in  generid  be  considered  to 
be  an  instrument  beyond  human  control.  The 
usages  of  Europe  have,  however,  made  it  the 
principal  instrument,  and,  when  it  is  present, 
have  reduced  all  others  to  an  accompaniment. 
Hence  it  is  necessary  that  these  other  instru- 
ments should  have  their  compass  and  pitch 
regulated  by  that  of  the  human  voice.  Now 
the  voice,  like  the  viol  family,  represents  at 
least  four  different  instruments — soprano,  alto, 
tenor,  and  bass,  with  two  intermediate  ones, 

m  mezzo-soprano,  between  soprano  and  alto,  and 
barytone,  between  tenor  and  bass.  It  is  there- 
fore as  necessary  to  determine  the  average  and 
exceptional  compass  of  these  species  of  voice 
as  it  is  to  know  the  compass  of  any  other 
instrument,  in  order  that  composers  may  be 
certain  as  to  what  sounds  can  be  reproduced, 
and  not  demand  any  other.  To  do  this,  the 
precise  acoustic  meaning  of  each  written  musi- 
cal note  should  be  ascertained.  The  difficulty 
of  determining  it  has  been  shown  by  the  pre- 
ceding history  of  musical  pitch  (pp.  494-513)1 
from  which,  combined  with  the  tauies  of  mean- 
tone  and  equal  intonation  (pp.  434  and  437),  it 
is  evident  that  Handel's  sustained  a"  in  the 
HdlUlvjah  chorus  had  845  vib.,  but  would 
now  be  sung  to  904  vib. ;  and  that  Mozart's 
/"  in  the  ZauberJWte  would  have  meant  1349 

CI  vib.,  but  would  now  have  to  be  sung  at  t^SS 
vib.  The  strain  that  this  would  put  upon 
voices  is  evident,  and  no  composer  who  wished 
his  music  to  be  well  represented  would  think 
of  making  such  demands  on  his  singers.  It 
appeared,  therefore,  necessary  to  ascertain 
more  precisely  than  had  been  hitherto  done, 
and  to  express  in  numbers  of  vibrations,  the 
limits  of  the  different  kinds  of  voices.  If  the 
composer  will  then  only  translate  his  written 
notes  into  numbers  of  vibrations,  by  the  table 
on  p.  437,  according  to  the  pitch  he  employs, 
he  will  avoid  all  danger  of  straining  singers. 

Through  the  kindness  and  liberality  of  the 
choir  conductors  Messrs.  Henry  Leslie,  W. 
G.  McNaught,  J.  Proudman,  Ebenezer  Prout, 
L.  C.  &  G.  J.  Venables,  and  542  members  of 
the  choirs  they  conducted,  I  was  able  to 
examine  a  sufficient  number  of  singers,  in 
January,  1S80,  to  arrive  at  something  like  a 


trustworthy  account  of  the  compass  of  the 
voice.    I  gave  each  singer  a  paper  with  the 
words  do  re  mi  fa  sol  la  ti  do'  printed  on  them 
in  four  columns  up  and  down  to  the  requisite 
extent,  and  then  started  them  on  (io  in  4 
different  pitches,  507,  522*5,  528,  5407  rib. 
(representing  the  just  cf^  corresponding  to  a' 
422-5   Handel's    pitch,  a'  435-4  the  French 
pitch,  a'  440  Scheibler's  pitoh,  and  the  equal 
c"  of  a'  4547i  the  highest  Philharmonic  pitch 
of  1874,  respectively).     I  got  them  to  sing  up 
and  down  in  chorus  under  the  direction  d 
the  conductor,  and  to  mark  with  a  pencil  the 
highest  and  lowest  note  eaoh  one  could  reach, 
first  easily  t  or  secondly  by  an  effort  (falsetto 
of  male  voices  being  exduded   in   the  first 
case,  but  not  in  the  last).    From  these  papers 
I  determined  by  calculation,  on  the  assump- 
tion of  just  intonation  (as  being  most  pro« 
bable  for  unaccompanied  singers),  the  numbers 
of  vibrations  in  the  limiting  notes.    These  are 
contained  in  the  following  table,  together  with 
the  mean  height  and  depth  of  all  the  voices. 
The  extreme  highest  limit  for  male  voices,  as 
it  included  falsetto,  is  a  mere  curiosity.    For 
writing  music,  the  mean  should  not  be  as- 
sumed as  the  limit,  for  perhaps  half  the  chorus 
could  not  reach  it.    But  it  would  be  perfectly 
safe  to  write  from  the  highest  low  easy  limit 
to  the  lowest  high  easy  limit.    Thus,  for  so- 
pranos it  would  not  do  to  write  up  to  b"  993 
and  down  to  /  180,  but  it  would  be  quite  safe 
to  write  up  to  /'  704  and  down  to  6  253. 
Viewed  in  this  way,  my  results  agree  more 
nearly  with  Randegger's,  which  I  add  for  com- 
parison.    These  last  are    given  in  a  staff- 
notation  form  in  his  primer  on  Singing  (No- 
vello,  1879)  1  uid  as  he  politely  informed  me 
that  he  assumed  Broadwood's  medium  pitch 
a'  446*2, 1  was  able  to  calculate  the  vibrations. 
All  the  numbers  of  vibrations  are  given  to  the 
nearest  integer  only,  and  it  is  to  these  numbers 
that  attention  should  be  especially  paid,  the 
names  of  the  notes  being  merely  guides.  Those 
letters  preceded  by  a  turned  period  relate  to 
high  pitch  in  the  column  *  Actual,'  and  those 
not  so  preceded  relate  to  a  medium  pitch, 
as  French  or  German.    But  in  the  column 
*  Mean  '  no  precise  system  at  all  could  be 
selected.      In  Randegger's,  of  course.  Broad- 
wood's  medium  pitch  is  intended.     If,  how- 
ever, the  notes  be  played  on  any  ordinary  piano, 
they  will  seldom  be  in  error  to  the  extent  ctf 
a  quarter  of  a  Tone. 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


,^OT.  N. 


MISCELLANEOUS  NOTES. 
Mean  akd  Actual  Coupasb  of  the  Huuam  Voice. 


545 


'VoicEB  Observed 

Easy  Lowkr  Limit 

VOICM  Obseryso 

KxTiisMx  Lower  LiMn* 

Mean 

Actual 

•6    253  to 

•^    13s 
ab  211  to 
c      132 
e     163  to 
D    76 
Ab  106  to 
C      66 

Mean 

Actual 

X46  Sopranos    . 

91  Altos  . 
X07  Tenors 
X  25  Basses 

/     180 
eb  161 
Q     98 
E     81 

173  Sopranos    . 
108  Altos  . 
114  Tenors 
140  Basses 

eb  162 
d    147 
E     85 
C«   72 

g   203  to 
c     130 
g    198  to 
B    124 
'B  127  to 
C     66 
'F    90  to 
•^.    56 

VOICSB  Obsertkd 

Bast  Hiohkr  Ldcit 

BXTRJCME  HlQHKR  LnflT 

Mean 

6-    993 
9"«  836 

C"      S2I 

/'«  375 

Actual 

Mean 

Actnal 

145  Sopranos    . 

83  Altos  . 
114  Tenors 
120  Basses 

/'"    1408  to 

r    704 

'd'"  1216  to 
•e"     676 
d"      608  to 

•c"      541  to. 
d'      294 

173  Sopranos    . 
105  Altos . 
112  Tenors 
139  Basses 

c"'Z  1 124 
.6"b    952 
d"      617 
6'b     4S3 

'a'"b  1690  to 

y'       811 

i^"    1584  to 

•r   721 

Y      8uto 
^'       396 
•c'"    io8i  to 
«'        330 

Bandkooeb's  Statement  of  LimnNo  Tones. 

VOICSB 

Reoi 

TLAR 

V01CE8 

BXCKPTIOKAL 

Lower  Limit 

Upper  Limit 

Lower  Limit 

Upper  Limit 

Soprano    . 
Mezzo  Soprano 
Alto  . 
Tenor 
Barytone  . 
Bass . 

6  b    236 

g     199 

$        167 

c      133 
A\>   105 
F      89 

c'"    1061 
6"b     945 
/"       708 
6'b     473 

/;    354 

eb     316 

Soprano    . 
Mezzo  Soprano 
Alto  . 
Tenor 
Barytone  . 
Bass 

6b  236 

g   199 

e     167 

F      87 
D     75 

/'"    1417 
c"'    io6i 

C     795 
c^«     562 

i    398 
/    354 

IT 


Harmonics  and  Partials  of  a  Pianoforte  String  struck  at  one-eighth 

of  its  length. 


On  p.  77,  note* ,  will  be  found  Mr.  Hipkins's 
observations  on  the  striking-point  of  piano- 
forte strings,  shewing  that  one-seventh  of  the 
length,  which  seemed  to  be  assumed  as  usual 
by  Prof.  Helmholtz,  was  not  in  use  generally, 
or  (p.  y6d')  at  Steinways*.  Prof.  Helmholtz 
conceived  that  the  origin  of  this  presumed 
custom  was  to  get  rid  of  the  7  th  partial, 
which  he  also  considered  likely  to  injure  the 
quality  of  tone.  Mr.  Hipkins's  experiments 
were  therefore  made  with  the  object  of  deter- 
mining whether  when  the  striking-place  was 
one  of  the  nodes  the  corresponding  partial 
disappeared,  as  results  from  the  mathematical 
formula  (12a)  supdL,  p.  3836. 

^  Mr.  Hipkins's  first  experiments  are  de- 
tailed in  his  paper  entitled  *  Observations  on 
the  Harmonics  of  a  String,  struck  at  one-eighth 
its  length  '  {Proc.  Royal  Society,  20  Nov.  1884, 
Yol.  zxxvii.  p.  363).  The  main  facts  are  given 
supr&,  p.  78^.  The  results  were  all  witnessed 
by  Dr.  Huggins,  F.B.S.,  and  myself.  The 
string  was  exactly  45  inches  long,  and  was 
struck  at  precisely  one-eighth  its  length  from 
the  wrestplank -bridge  (that  nearest  the  player). 
When  it  was  touched  with  a  piece  of  felt  at 
5*63,  i6'88,  and  28*13  inches  from  the  belly- 
bridge  (that  farthest  from  the  player),  which 


are  three  positions  of  the  nodes  for  the  8tlv 
partial  or  third  Octave  higher,  selected  to  avoid 
errors  (as  not  being  positions  of  the  nodes  of 
the  2nd  or  4th  partials),  in  each  case  the  8th 
harmonic  was  well  heard.  It  was  not  so 
strong  as  the  4th,  5th,  6th,  7th,  and  9th,  all 
of  which  were  heard,  but  quite  unmistakable, 
and  was  heard  better  on  removing  the  felt 
immediately  after  the  note  had  been  pro-  «r, 
duced.  The  i6th  partial  was  also  heard  when  ^' 
the  string  was  touched  at  its  nodes  2-81  and 
8'44  inches  from  the  belly-bridge,  which  are 
nodes  of  the  i6th  hat  not  of  the  8th  partial. 

What  was  heard  was  the  harmonic^  not  the 
simple  partial  tone,  and  it  was  suggested,  that 
perhaps  touching  the  string  at  the  node  coerced 
the  string  and  obliged  it  to  vibrate  with  these 
nodes,  notwithstanding  that  it  was  struck  in 
one  of  the  series  of  such  nodes.  Mr;  Hipkins, 
therefore,  at  my  suggestion  made  a  new  series 
of  experiments,  detailed  in  his  paper  entitled 
*  Observations  on  the  Upper  Partial  Tones  of 
a  Pianoforte  String,  struck  at  one-eighth  its 
length  *  (Proc,  of  the  Royal  Society ,  15  Jan. 
1885,  vol.  xxxviii.  p.  83).  These  experiments 
I  also  witnessed.  The  object  was  to  leave  the 
string  perfectly  uneoerceid,  and  to  avoid  the 
use  of  resonators,  on  which  some  suspicion  had 


Digitized  by 


(Jbogle 


546 


ADDITIONS  BY  THE  TRANSLATOE. 


APP.  XX. 


been  (wrongly,  as  I  believe)  oast  by  at  least 
one  observer.  Calculating  the  pitch  of  the 
partials,  which  would  be  the  same  as  that  of 
the  harmonics,  and  the  interval  which  the 
tempered  notes  of  the  piano  would  make  with 
them  (as  in  Table  II.,  supriL,  p.  457),  a  string 
of  the  corresponding  note  was  slackened  (or 
tightened,  as  convenient ;  sometimes  both  al- 
ternately), while  the  other  unison  strings  were 
damped  with  the  usual  tuners'  wedges ;  and  in 
the  same  way  only  one  of  the  three  lower 
strings  was  allowed  to  vibrate.  Then  the  low 
note  and  the  high  note  were  struck  simul- 
taneously. It  is  evident  that  the  high  note 
being  shghtly  out  of  unison  with  the  upper 
partial  of  the  lower  note,  beats  would  ensue  if 
such  a  partial  existed.  Now,  for  the  5th,  6th, 
^  7th,  8th,  9th,  loth,  and  i  ith  partial  such  beats 
were  perfectly  audible,  but  their  duration  for 
ithe  nth  partial  with  1487  vib.  was  so  short 
ihat  higher  ones  were  not  tried.  For  the  8th 
partial  the  beats  were  quite  distinct,  and,  on 
removing  the  wedges  that  damped  the  unison 
strings  for  the  high  note,  and  striking  the  three 
high  strings  without  the  lower  note,  it  was 
evident  that  the  beats  heard  were  the  same  in 
rapidity  and  character  as  when  the  single 
string  was  sounded  with  the  low  one. 

The  fact,  therefore,  that  the  8th  partial 
existed  was  conclusivelv  proved.  Various 
causes  have  been  assigned.  On  p.  383^,  note  *, 
I  have  suggested  that  if  terms  omitted  by  the 
hypothesis  named  were  introduced,  perhaps 
there  would  be  a  residuum  which  would  account 
for  hearing  the  8th  partial.  This  partial  was 
mr  really  much  weaker  than  the  7th.  The  last, 
''  indeed,  was  quite  clear  and  ringing,  so  that  it 
did  not  seem  affected  by  striking  the  string  so 
near  its  node. 

It  is  curious  that  when  the  nodes  do  not 
lie  Very  close  the  harmonic  could  be  brought 
out  by  touching  the  string  somewhat  near  the 
proper  place.  Thus  for  the  2nd  harmonic, 
node  at  22*5  inches  from  the  belly-bridge ;  the 
next  nodes  were  1*2  inch  nearer  and  1*2  inch 
farther  from  that  spot,  and  on  trial  the  2nd 
harmonic  or  Octave  came  out  when  the  string 
was  touched  between  22-1  and  22*95  inches 


from  the  belly-bridge,  but  not  at  22*05  and  23*0, 
so  there  was  a  *  play  '  of  '85  inch.  For  the 
3rd  harmonic  there  was  similarly  a  *  play '  of 
'65  inch.  A  very  remarkable  fact  was  that  by 
stopping  within  1*5  inch  of  the  belly-bridge,  the 
simple  prime  or  lowest  partial  came  out  un- 
accompanied by  any  other  audible  partials. 
This  was  tested  by  beats  of  forks,  shewing  that 
the  2nd  and  4th  partials  did  not  exist. 

Various  causes  for  the  sounding  of  the  8th 
harmonic  have  been  suggested.  One  of  these 
was  that  the  hammer  of  the  pianoforte,  being 
round  and  soft,  did  not  strike  at  one  point,  and 
so  excited  the  string  on  each  side  of  the  node. 
To  avoid  this  action  the  much  harder  hammer 
of  the  highest  note  (A  in  the  3-inch  octave)  was 
used  in  supplementary  experiments  made  on 
2  April  1885.  The  width  of  the  part  of  the 
hammer  that  came  in  contact  with  the  string 
did  not  exceed  ^  inch.  And  again,  an  ivoty 
edge,  not  more  than  ^  inch  in  width,  was  used 
instead  of  the  felt  covering  of  the  hammer. 
The  8th  partial,  tried  by  brats,  in  both  cases 
came  out  much  stronger  than  before,  and  the 
beats  could  be  distinctly  heard  10  or  12  feet  off. 
Again,  it  was  supposed  that  the  string  might 
not  be  uniform,  and  that  if  the  strikio^-plaoe 
were  slightly  moved  from  the  theoretical  node, 
an  actual  node  would  be  reached,  and  the  par- 
tial quenched.  Hence  the  ivory  head  of  the 
hanmier  was  shifted  so  as  to  strike  up  to  ^ 
inch  away  from  the  node  on  either  side.  The 
partial  was  heard  strongly,  but  the  sound  oi 
the  note  was  not  so  pleasant  as  when  the  string 
was  struck  at  the  actual  node. 

It  has  been  also  suggested  that  the  string 
moved  the  points  of  support,  but  that  we  had 
no  means  of  testing.  The  phenomenon,  there- 
fore, remains  unexplained ;  but  thanks  to  Mr. 
Hipkins,  who  had  the  resources  of  Broadwoods' 
establishment  and  the  assistance  of  expe- 
rienced tuners  at  command,  there  is  no  doubt 
whatever  of  the  fact,  that  a  pianoforte  string 
when  struck  at  a  node  by  a  hard  or  soft  ham- 
mer does  not  lose  the  corresponding  partial, 
and  does  not  materially  enfeeble  the  partials 
with  adjacent  nodes. 


3.  History  of  Meantone  TemperamenL 


This  is  the  temperament  usually,  but 
wrongly,  known  as  *  unequal '  (supr&,  p.  434a'), 
which  prevailed  so  long  over  Europe,  and  is 

-  not  yet  entirely  extruded. 

M  Arnold  Sohlick,  Spiegel  der  Orgelmacher 
irh  Oraanisten  (Mirror  of  Organ-builders  and 
Orgamsts),  1511,  chap,  viii.,  orders  the  Fifths 
jPC,  CO,  OD,  DA  to  be  tuned  as  flat  as  the 
ear  could  bear,  so  as  to  make  the  major  Third 
FA  decent.  Then  he  tunes  AE,  BD  in  the 
same  manner.  Beginning  again  with  F,  he 
tunes  the  Fifths  down  FBb,  Bb  Eb,  sharpen- 
ing  the  lower  note  for  the  same  reason.  Then 
he  tunes  EbAb  and  makes  ^ b  *  not  sharp,  but 
somewhat  flatter  than  the  Fifth  requires,  on 
account  of  the  proof  (vmb  das  brifen),  although, 
however,  the  Gt  thus  made  is  never  a  good 
Third  or  perfect  Sixth  to  the  Fifth  E  a.nA  Bt^ 
for  cadences  in  A.*  He  prides  himself,  how- 
ever, on  the  Ab  or  G8  ,  and  shews  how  to  dis- 
guise inaccuracies.  And  he  refutes  those  who 
would  make  Ot  good  for  cadences  in  ii  in  the 
chord  E  Gff  B,  by  saying  this  produces  weak- 


ness, and  takes  away  the  effect  of  good  and 
strange  consonances.  For  the  rest,  he  tunes 
B  FZ  with  the  upper  note  flat,  and  apparently 
FH  Ct  in  the  same  way.  This  was  really  an 
unequal  temperament,  and  looks  very  like  the 
meantone  temperament  spoiled,  but  that  sys- 
tem was  not  yet  discovered.  Schlick's  editor 
(Bob.  Eitner,  in  the  MonaUhefU  der  Mttsik- 
OeschichUt  monthly  parts  of  the  History  of 
Music,  part  I.,  1869)  says  that  what  Schliok 
claims  for  Ab  and  Ot  was  supposed  to  be  the 
invention  of  Barth.  Fritz  of  Brunswick  in  1756, 
245  years  later. 

Giuseppe  Zarlino  of  Chioggia,  50  yetrs 
after  Schlick,  in  his  Le  istilvtioni  harmoniche 
(Institutes  of  Harmony),  Venice,  1562,  speaks 
of  alcuni  (some  people)  who  seemed  to  think 
that  the  interval  of  the  comma  should  be  dis- 
tributed among  the  two  nearest  intervals,  and 
the  others  left  in  their  natural  foim  (cap.  43« 
p.  128).  This  would  give  a  meantone  for  the 
second  of  the  major  scale  =}x  (204-1- 182)" 
193  oentR,  but  leave  the  others  very  dissonant. 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


SECT.  N. 


MISCELLANEOUS  NOTES. 


547 


and  to  this  Zarlino  rightly  objects.  It  would 
give  the  major  scale  Co,  2>I93,  ^3^*  ^49^, 
O702,  ASS^j  B1088,  CI200  cents,  so  that  the 
Fourtii  D  :  O  would  have  509  cents,  and  the 
Tilth  D  :  A  would  have  691  cents,  which  com- 
ing in  the  midst  of  just  intervals  would  be 
intolerable,  and  beyond  the  natural  key  it  fails 
entirely.  Zarlino's  remedy  (chap.  42,  p.  126) 
is  to  diminish  every  Fifth  by  two-sevenths  of 
a  comma,  and  he  proceeds  to  shew  how  this 
affects  the  tuning.  It  preserves  the  small 
Semitone  24  :  25  «■  70*673  cents.  He  says, 
p.  127,  *  although  in  instruments  thus  tem- 
pered consonances  cannot  be  given  in  their 
perfect — that  is,  their  true  and  natural  form — 
yet  they  can  be  used  when  the  chords  have 
to  be  given  in  their  true  and  natural  propor- 
tions. I  say  this,'  he  adds,  *  because  I  have 
frequently  made  the  experiment  on  an  instru- 
ment which  I  had  made  for  the  purpose,  and 
the  effect  may  be  tried  on  any  other  instru- 
ment, especially  the  harpsichord  and  clavi- 
chord, which  are  well  adapted  for  the  purpose.* 
Then,  in  chap.  43,  he  proceeds  to  shew  that 
this  temperament  is  rationally  constructed, 
and  that  no  other  is  so  {che  per  altro  modo 
non  H  possa  fare,  that  is,  ragiofieuolmente). 
It  is  quite  clear,  then,  that  Zarlino,  as  has 
often  been  asserted,  did  not  invent  the  mean- 
tone  temperament,  and  did  not  consider  equal 
temperament  worth  mentioning,  even  if  he  was 
acquainted  with  it. 

Francis  Salinas  of  Burgos  in  Spain,  bom 
1 513,  died  1590,  blind  from  infancy.  Professor 
of  Music  in  the  University  of  Salamanca, 
Abb6  of  St.  Pancras  do  Bocca  Scalegno,  in 
the  kingdom  of  Naples,  in  1577  published  his 
De  Muaica  libri  septem^  of  which  a  very  im- 
perfect, and,  as  respects  temperament,  in- 
correct account  is  given  in  Bumey  &  Haw- 
kins. Salinas  says  (lib.  iii.  cap.  zv.  p.  143, 
I  translate  his  Latin) : — 

'  From  what  has  been  said,  in  order  that 
Tones  should  be  rendered  equal,  the  minor 
must  be  increased  and  the  major  diminished. 
It  must  be  observed  that  this  can  be  done  in 
several  ways,  because  the  comma,  by  which 
they  differ,  may  be  divided  in  many  ways. 
Of  these,  three  have  been  thought  out  up  to 
this  time,  which  seem  to  me  most  suitable 
{aptissimi).  Hence  arise  three  ways  of  tem- 
pering imperfect  instruments.  The  first  is 
to  divide  the  comma  into  three  proportional 
parts,  giving  one  to  the  minor  Tone,  and  tak- 
ing two  from  the  major  Tone.  This  gives  a 
new  Tone,  larger  than  the  minor  and  smaller 
than  the  major.  The  decrement  is  twice  the 
increment,  and  through  the  maximum  in- 
eqnality  the  tone  becomes  equal.* 

The  comma  has  21*506  cents,  hence 
^  comma  has  7*169,  and  )  comma  has 
14*388  cents.  Then  182*404-1- 7*169"  189*572 
=  203*910-14*338  cents,  which  is  what  the 
above  statement  comes  to,  giving  189*572 
cents  for  the  new  Tone.  Notwi&standing 
this  very  precise  statement,  Salinas  ought  to 
mean  precisely  the  reverse.  His  object  was  to 
make  tiie  Tritone  perfect,  and  to  make  it  con- 
sist of  three  new  Tones.  Now  a  Tritone  F  :  B 
consists  of  2  major  Tones  and  i  minor  Tone, 
— that  is,  3  minor  Tones  and  2  commas,  or 
590*224  cents,  ^  of  which  is  196*741  cents, 
which  is  182*404+14*338  and  203*910-7*169 


—that  is,  the  reverse  of  the  former  result.  By 
a  singular  error  perpetuated  in  a  figure  (which, 
of  course,  being  blind  he  could  not  see),  Salinas 
makes  the  Tritone  in  Hhis  place  consist  of 

2  minor  Tones  and  i   major  Tone — that  is, 

3  minor  Tones  and  i  comma,  having  the  ratio 
18  :  25,  or  cents  568-718,  which  is  not  the 
Tritone,  but  the  superfluous  Fourth,  and  may 
here  be  called  the  false  Tritone.  This  mis- 
take seems  to  have  arisen  thus.  The  Octave, 
as  he  rightly  says,  has  6  minor  Tones,  2 
commas,  and  a  great  DiSsis.  *  The  comma 
being  divided  into  3  proportional  parts,  if  one 
is  added  to  each  minor  Tone,  2  commas  will 
be  added  to  the  six  Tones,  and  one  to  three, 
equally  distributed  among  them.  From  which 
distribution  it  will  follow  in  this  constitution 
of  the  temperament  that  the  Tritone  consists 
of  three  minor  Tones  and  one  comma,  or  f 
2  minor  Tones  and  one  major,*  whence  he 
deduces  the  ratio  18  :  25.  But  he  thus  alto- 
gether loses  sight  of  the  great  DiSsis,  and 
considers  a  Tritone  to  be  half  of  an  Octave 
after  it  has  been  diminished  by  a  Digsis.  On 
p.  155  he  again  notices  the  Tritone  as  32  :  45, 
the  correct  ratio.  The  false  Tritonic  tempera- 
ment therefore  makes  the  Tone  189*572,  the 
Fifth  694-786,  and  the  false  Tritone  568*718 
cents.  But  the  true  Tritonic  system  gives  the 
Tone  of  196-74,  the  Fifth  of  698*37  and  the 
true  Tritone  590^22  cents. 

Salinas  continues  his  account  of  the  three 
temperaments  thus :  '  The  second  [tempera- 
ment] divides  the  comma  into  7  proportional 
parts,  giving  3  to  the  minor  and  taking  4  from 
the  major  Tone.*  This  is  Zarlino's  tempera- 
ment already  described,  and  preserves  the  m 
small  Semitone  24  :  25  »  70*673  cents.  '  The 
third  will  arise  from  halving  the  comma, 
giving  half  to  the  minor  and  taJcing  half  from 
the  major  Tone.*  Then  he  adds  (p.  164) : 
*  Wherefore  any  one  of  these  three  tempsraments 
seems  most  suitable  for  artificial  instruments  ; 
nor  have  any  more  been  as  yet  thought  out 
(neque  plwra  adhuc  excogitata  sunt) ;  *  that  is, 
Salinas,  like  Zarlino,  utterly  ignores  the  equal 
temperament.  *The  first,  so  far  as  I  know, 
has  been  laid  down  by  no  one.*  From  which 
it  is  to  be  inferred  that  it  was  his  own  inven- 
tion. *  The  second  I  have  also  found  in  the 
harmonic  institutions  of  Joseph  Zarlino  of 
Ghioggia,*  as  already  given.  *  The  third  was 
commenced,  but  not  perfected,  by  Luigi  or 
Ludovico  Folliano  of  M6dena,*  who  must  have 
been  Zarlino's  *  some  people  *  (alcuni),  *  And  ^ 
Joseph  Zarlino  has  properly  considered  it  in 
his  harmonic  demonstrations.  But  no  one 
has  previously  acknowledged  all  three,  nor 
observed  upon  their  relation  and  mutuiU 
order.* 

It  was  Salinas  who  finished  Folliano*s 
work,  and  in  chaps.  22  to  25  he  describes 
the  result  thoroughly.  As,  therefore,  we  con- 
sider Watt,  and  not  the  Marquis  of  Worces- 
ter, to  have  invented  the  steam  engine,  we 
must  consider  Salinas,  and  not  Folliano,  to 
have  invented  the  meantone  temperament.  I 
^ve  a  comparative  table  of  all  three  schemes 
m  cents  to  the  nearest  integer,  from  Eb  to 
OZ  •  distinguishing  the  true  and  false  Tritonic 
and  adding  the  Equal,  which  will  shew  the  real 
rdbibtions  of  these  three  temperaments  to  each 
other. 


Digitized  by 


G5bgle 


548 


ADDITIONS  BY  THE  TRANSLATOB. 


AFP.  XX. 


Notes 

True  Tritonic 

Palse  Tritonic 

Zflrllno 

Ueantone 

Equal 

c 

O 

0 

0 

0 

0 

c» 

89 

64 

71 

76 

100 

D 

197 

190 

192 

193 

200 

Eb 

305 

316 

313 

310 

300 

E 

393 

379 

383 

386 

400 

F 

502 

50s 

504 

503 

500 

Fti 

S90 

569 

575 

580 

600 

G 

698 

695 

696 

697 

700 

GZ 

787 

758 

766 

773 

800 

A 

89s 

884 

887 

890 

900 

Bb 

1003 

lOIO 

1009 

1007 

1000 

B 

1092 

1074 

1079 

1083 

1 100 

c 

1200 

1200 

1200 

1200 

1200 

ir  The  true  Tritonio,  making  the  Tritone 
590-22  cents,  necessarily  differs  very  slightly 
from  equal  temperament,  which  makes  it  600 
cents,  while  the  false  Tritonic,  making  the 
Tritone  568716,  or  a  comma  too  flat,  ap- 
proaches very  near  to  Zarlino's  and  the  Mean- 


tone,  BO  that  I  think  Salinas  must  have 
intended  to  use  this  one;  which  he  lays  down 
so  clearly,  and  that  he  accidentally  made  a 
mistake  of  a  comma  in  estimating  the  Tritone, 
by  hastily  neglecting  the  DiSsiB.  For  later 
usages  see  suprd,,  pp.  320,  321. 


4,  The  History  of  Equal  Temperament, 


When  once  the  Pythagorean  division  of 
the  Octave  had  been  settled,  and  it  had  been 
observed  that  12  Fifths  exceeded  7  Octaves  by 
the  small  interval  of  a  Pythagorean  comma 
(p.  432,  art.  9),  the  idea  of  distributing  this 
error  among  the  12  Fifths  was  obvious. 
Aristoxenus,  a  pupil  of  Aristotle,  the  son  of 
a  musician  and  a  writer  on  music,  is  said  to 
have  advocated  this.  At  any  rate  he  stated 
that  the  Fourth  consisted  of  two  Tones  and  a 

^  half,  which  is  exactly  true  only  in  equal  tem- 
perament. Amiot  reports  equal  temperament 
from  China  long  previously  even  to  Pytha- 
goras. In  later  times  Mersenne  {Harmonie 
Universellej  1636)  gives  the  correct  numbers 
for  the  ratios  of  equal  temperament,  and  says 
(Livre  3,  prop.  xii.  '  Des  genres  de  la  musique  *) 
of  equal  temperament  that  it  *  est  le  pita 
usiU  et  le  plus  commode,  et  que  tous  les  prao- 
ticiens  avoiient  que  la  division  de  TOctave  en 
12  demitons  leur  est  plus  facile  pour  toucher 
les  instruments.*  This  should  imply  that 
there  were  numerous  instruments  in  «qual 
temperament,  but  I  have  not  been  able  to  find 
any  noticed.  BMos  (L'Art  dufacteur  d^Orgues, 
1766)  knows  only  meantone  temperament, 
which  he  gives  directions  for  tuning.  In  Ger- 
many, Werckmeister  (Orgelprobe,  2nd  edit. 
1698)  says  that  he  can  only  recommend  equal 

«r  temperament,  and  Schnitger  of  Harburg  in 
Hanover,  and  afterwards  of  Hamburg,  an  ad- 
mirer of  Werckmeister,  built  the  organ  of  St. 
Jacobi-Kirche  in  Hamburg  in  1688,  and  tuned 
it  in  intentionally  equal  temperament.  Herr 
Schmahl,  who  had  been  the  organist  there 
since  1838,  never  knew  it  otherwise  tuned, 
and  could  find  no  record  of  any  change  of 
intonation  in  the  archives  of  the  church,  and 
he  also  could  not  recollect  having  ever  heard 
of  any  other  intonation  in  North  Germany. 
His  master,  Demuth  (died  1848)  of  St.  Catha- 
rinen-Kirche,  whose  memory  extended  back- 
wards to  iSio,  also  knew  of  no  other  tuning 
in  North  Germany.  Of  course  the  tempera- 
ment never  was  thoroughly  equal,  so  that 
when  Herr  Schmahl  practised  on  the  St. 
Catharine's  organ,  the  usual  keys  C  and  G 
were  not  sic  good  as  the  unusual  keys  FU  and 


D8.  Dr.  Robert  Smith,  1759,  must  have 
heard  equal  temperament,  or  else  he  could 
hardly  have  spoken  of  *that  inharmonious 
system  of  12  hemitones*  producing  a  'har- 
mony extremely  coarse  and  disagreeable ' 
{Harmonics f  2nd  ed.  pp.  i66>7),  but  it  may 
have  been  only  an  experimental  instrument  of 
his  own. 

As  regards  the  recent  introduction  of  equal 
temperament  into  England,  Mr.  James  Broad- 
wood,  in  the  New  Monthly  Mctganne^  i  Sept 
z8i I,  proposed  it,  and  gave  the  error  of  the 
Fifths  as  ^  Semitone  (  =  2^  cents),  which  was 
to  him  the  smallest  sensible  intervaL     On 

1  Oct.  181 1,  Mr.  John  Farey,  sen.,  shewed  that 
this  was  too  much  (it  should  be  1*954,  or  about 

2  cents — that  is,  about  -^^  Semitone),  and  re- 
ferred to  the  article  *  Equal  Temperament '  in 
Bees's  Cyclopedia.  Hereupon,  on  i  Nov.  181 1, 
Mr.  James  Broadwood  rejoined  that  be  gave 
merely  a  practical  method  of  producing  equal 
temperament,  *  from  its  being  in  most  general 
use,  and  because  of  the  various  systems  it  has 
been  pronounced  the  best  deserving  that  ap- 
pellation by  Haydn,  Mozart,  and  other  masters 
of  harmony.'  ITnfortunately  he  gives  no  refer- 
ences, and  consequently  this  assertion  can  be 
taken  only  as  an  unverified  impression.  Haydn 
died  1808,  Mozart  1791,  but  the  Hamburg 
organs  had  equal  temperament  long  before 
that  time.  Sebastian  Bach  (died  1750)  is  gene- 
rally credited  with  introducing  equal  tem- 
perament, but  M.  Bosanquet  says  *  Uiere  is  no 
direct  evidence  that  he  ever  played  upon  an 
organ  tuned  according  to  equal  temperament ' 
(Musical  Intervals  and  Temperament^  1876, 
p.  31).  Bitter,  however,  states,  in  his  life  of 
Sebastian  Bach,  that  he  once  played  on  the  St. 
Jacobi  organ  at  Hamburg,  and  expressed  his 
approval  of  the  tuning,  and  even  applied  for 
the  post  of  organist.  The  wohl  temperirtes 
Clavier^  or  well-tuned  clavichord,  the  notes  of 
which  are  very  fugitive,  was  the  instniment 
mentioned  by  Carl  Philip  Emanuel  Bach,  who 
died  1788. 

As  regards  Mr.  James  Broadwood  *8  state- 
ment that  equal  temperament  was  in  181 1  *in 
most  general  use  '  —presumably  in  England-- 


Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


SECT.  N. 


MISCELLANEOUS  NOTES. 


549 


'h/lr.  Hipkins  has  been  at  some  pains  to  ascer- 

XsLin  how  far  that  was  the  case,  and  from  him 

X  learn  that  Mr.  Peppercorn,  who  tuned  origin- 

aJ.ly  for  the  Philharmonic  Society,  was  concert 

'tuner  at  Broadwoods',  and  a  great  favoarite  of 

Bf  r.  James  Broadwood.    His  son  writes  to  Mr. 

Bjpkins  that  his  father  *  always  tnned  so  that 

will  keys  can  be  played  in,  and  neither  he  nor 

I   [neither  father  nor  son]  ever  held  with  making 

some  keys  sweet  and  others  sour.*    Mr.  Bailey, 

liowever,  who  succeeded  Mr.  Peppercorn  as 

concert  tuner,  and  tuned  Mr.  James  Broad- 

VT'ood's  own  piano  at  Lyne,  his  country  house, 

used  the  meantone  temperament  to  Mr.  Hip- 

Icins's  own  knowledge,  and  no  other.    Not  one 

of  the  old  tuners  Mr.  Hipkins  knew  (and  some 

iiad  been  favourite  tuners  of  Mr.  James  Broad- 

^'ood)  tuned  anything  like  equal  temperament. 

Ck>llard,  the  Wilkies,  Challenger,  Seymour,  all 

timed  the  meantone  temperament,  except  that, 

like  Arnold  Schlick,  151 1  (see  p.  546<2),  they 

raised  the  Gt  somewhat  to  mitigate  the  *■  wolf  ' 

resulting  from  the  Fifth  Eb  :  GU  in  place  of 

£b  :  Ab.    Hence  Mr.  James  Broadwood    did 

not  succeed  in  introducing  equal  temperament 

permanently  even  into  his  own  establishment, 

and  all  tradition  of  it  died  out  long  ago.    So 

far  runs  Mr.  Hipkins's  interesting  information. 

In    181 2  Dr.  Crotch  (Elements  of  Musical 

Composition^    pp.     134-S)    gives    the  proper 

figures  for  equal  temperament,  shews  how  it 

arose,  that  its  Fifths  are  too  flat  and  its  major 

Thirds  too  sharp,  adding  *  this  will  render  all 

keys  equally  imperfect,'  but  says  nothing  to 

recommend  it.    Yet  in  1840  Dr.  Crotch  (who 

died  in  1847)  had    his  own  chamber  organ 

tuned  in  equal  temperament,  as  I  have  been 

informed  by  Mr.  £.  J.  Hopkins,  author  of  The 

Organ^  dtc. 

It  is  one  thing  to  propose  equal  tempera- 
ment, to  calculate  its  ratios,  and  to  have  trial 
instruments  approximately  tuned  in  accord- 
ance with  it,  and  another  thing  to  use  it  com- 
mercially in  all  instruments  sold.  For  pianos 
in  England  it  did  not  become  a  trade  usage 
till  1846,  at  about  which  time  it  was  intro- 
duced into  Broadwoods*  under  the  superintend- 
ence of  Mr.  Hipkins  himself.  At  least  eight 
years  more  elapsed  before  equal  temperament 
was  generally  used  for  organs,  on  which  its 


defects  are  more  apparent,  although  not  to  such 
an  extent  as  on  the  harmonium. 

In  1 85 1,  at  the  Great  Exhibition,  no  Eng- 
lish organ  was  tuned  in  equal  temperament, 
but  the  only  German  organ  exhibited  (Schulze's) 
was  so  tuned. 

In  July  1852  Messrs.  J.  W.  Walker  &  Sons 
put  their  Exeter  Hall  organ  into  equal  tem- 
perament, but  it  was  not  used  publicly  till 
November  of  that  year.  Meanwhile,  in  Sept., 
Mr.  George  Herbert,  a  barrister  and  amateur, 
then  in  charge  of  the  organ  in  the  Koman 
Catholic  Church  in  Farm  Street,  Berkeley 
Square,  London,  had  that  organ  tuned  equally 
by  Mr.  Hill,  the  builder.  Though  much  op- 
posed, it  was  visited  and  approved  by  many, 
and  among  others  by  Mr.  Cooper,  who  had  the 
organ  in  the  hall  of  Christ's  Hospital  (the 
Bluecoat  School)  tuned  equally  in  1853.  ^ 

In  1854  the  first  organ  built  and  tuned 
oiiginally  in  equal  temperament,  by  Messrs. 
Gray  &  Davison,  was  niade  for  Dr.  Eraser's 
Congregational  Chapel  at  Blackburn  (both 
chapel  and  organ  have  since  been  burned). 
In  the  same  year  Messrs.  Walker  and  Mr. 
Willis  sent  out  their  first  equally  tempered 
organs.  This  must  therefore  be  considered 
as  the  commercial  date  for  equal  temperament 
on  new  organs  in  England.  On  old  organs 
meantone  temperament  lingered  much  later. 
In  1880,  when  I  wrote  my  History  of  Musical 
Pitch,  from  which  most  of  these  particulars 
have  been  taken,  I  found  meantone  tempera- 
ment still  general  in  Spain,  and  used  in  Eng- 
land on  Greene's  three  organs,  at  St.  George's 
Chapel,  Windsor  (since  altered),  at  St.  Katha- 
rine's, Regent's  Park  (see  p.  484^/),  and  at 
Kew  Parish  Church  ;  and  while  many  others  If 
had  only  recently  been  altered,  one  (Jordan's 
at  Maidstone  Old  Parish  Church)  was  being 
altered  when  I  visited  it  in  that  year.  Hence, 
in  England,  equal  temperament,  though  now 
(1885)  firmly  established,  is  not  yet  quite  40 
years  old  on  the  pianoforte,  and  only  30 
years  old  on  the  organ. 

The  difficulty  of  tuning  in  equal  tempera- 
ment led  to  the  invention  of  Scheibler's  tuning- 
fork  tonometer.  In  Sect.  G.,  art.  11,  p.  489, 
will  be  found  a  practical  rule  for  tuning  in 
sensibly  equal  temperament  at  all  usual  pitches. 


5.  Professor  Mayer's  Analysis  of  Compound  Tones  and  Hariruynic  Curves, 


The  following  are  two  of  the  numerous 
acoustical  contrivances  of  Mr.  Alfred  M.  Mayer, 
Pb.D.,  Member  of  the  National  (American) 
Academy  of  Sciences,  and  Professor  of  Physics 
in  the  Stevens  Institute  of  Technology,  Hobo- 
ken,  New  Jersey,  United  States  (see  supr^,  p. 
417c). 

I.  New  Objective  Analysis  of  Compound 
Sounds.  The  analysis  of  compound  sounds  by 
resonators  has  two  disadvantages:  first,  that 
it  is  subjective,  inasmuch  as  but  one  observer 
at  a  time  is  capable  of  hearing  the  results ; 
and,  secondly,  that  the  range  of  pitch  reinforce- 
able  by  a  resonator  is  too  great  for  extreme 
accuracy  in  the  estimation  of  the  actual  com- 
ponent sound  present.  Both  of  these  disad- 
vantages were  thus  overcome  (Phil,  Mag,  Oct. 
1874,  vol.  xlviii.  pp.  271-3,  with  a  figure). 

A  Greni^'s  free-reed  pipe,  of  the  pitch 
C=i28,  had  part  of  its  wooden  chamber  re- 
moved and  replaced  by  morocco  leather,  at 


one  point  of  which  8  silk  cocoon  fibres  were 
attached,  having  their  opposite  extremities 
attached  to  8  tuning-forks  tuned  to  C,  c,  a,  c',  ^ 
ef  g't'b'bt  c",  at  the  point  of  the  upper  node  in 
each  where  it  divides  into  segments  when 
giving  its  upper  harmonic,  so  that  this  har- 
monic was  eliminated.  The  cocoon  fibres  were 
stretched  till  they  made  no  visible  ventral  seg- 
ments when  vibrating.  The  reed  was  tuned 
accurately  to  the  C  fork  (of  64  vibrations)  by 
means  of  the  g  fork.  The  forks  were  placed 
on  proper  resonance  boxes.  When  the  reed 
was  sounded  each  fork  *  sang  out  '  loudly,  but 
if  the  prongs  of  any  fork  were  only  slightly 
loaded  the  fork  was  mute,  ard  was  so  rapidly 
affected  that  Prof.  Mayer  estimates  (same  vol. 
p.  519)  that  the  effect  of  intervals  such  as 
2000  :  2001  (or  '87  or  not  quite  i  cent)  can  be 
rendered  sensible  to  the  ear.  On  ceasing  to 
sound  the  reed  the  forks  continued  to  sound, 
and  produced  a  tone  of  so  nearly  the  same 


Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


550 


ADDITIONS  BY  THE  TEANSLATOR. 


APP.  XX. 


quality  as  that  of  the  reed  that  it  was  easy  to 
feel  that  the  difference  was  due  to  the  absence 
of  partials  higher  than  the  eighth.  By  this 
means,  then,  the  analysis  and  synthesis  of  a 
compound  tone  can  be  shewn  to  a  large 
audience  at  once,  and  all  doubt  as  to  its  ob- 
jective reality  removed.  At  the  same  time  the 
air  in  the  resonance  chamber  of  the  reed  acts 
on  the  leather  cover  as  in  hearing  it  would 
have  acted  on  the  drumskin  of  the  ear,  and 
the  conduction  of  that  vibration  by  the  cocoon 
fibres  replaces  the  complicated  arrangements 
in  the  interior  of  the  drum  and  the  fluid  of  the 
labyrinth  of  the  ear,  while  the  forks  themselves 
serve  as  the  organs  in  the  cochlea.  Prof. 
Helmholtz's  physiological  theory  of  audition  is 
thus  perfectly  exhibited  in  a  *  working  model.* 
^  The  action  of  the  resonance  chambers  of  the 
forks  is  simply  to  make  the  effects  heard  at  a 
distance. 

2.  Harmonic  Curves  (see  p.  387^).  In  the 
Philosophical  Magazine^  Supplement  for  Jan- 
uary 1875,  vol.  xlviii.  pp.  520-525,  Prof.  Mayer 
gives  curves  compounded  by  six  curves  of  sines 
(p.  23d'),  representing  six  partial  tones,  where, 
for  convenience,  the  amplitudes  are  taken  to 
vary  as  the  wave  lengths,  and  to  have  the  same 
initial  phase.    They  are  combined  by  taking 


the  algebraical  sum  of  their  ordinates,  which 
law  would  of  course  not  hold  true  for  the  am- 
plitudes chosen  (about  one-third  of  the  length 
of  the  wave).  The  resulting  figure  bears  a 
most  remarkable  resemblance  to  fig.  25,  supr^, 
p.  846.  There  is  the  same  sudden  rise  on  the 
left  and  step-like  descent  on  the  right,  but 
the  steps  are  more  rounded,  and  the  upper 
crest  more  pointed,  and  there  are  five  steps  in 
addition  to  the  crest.  Prof.  Mayer  then  com- 
bines two  such  compound  curves,  and  thus 
produces  the  resulting  curves  of  two  oompoond 
tones  forming  an  Octave  (with  one  high  and 
one  low  crest,  and  also  one  high  and  one  low 
trough,  and  the  steps  uneven  and  reduced  in 
number),  a  Fifth  (with  four  crests,  two  with 
long  and  two  with  short  descents,  the  shorter 
having  only  one  step),  and  a  major  Third  (with 
eight  crests,  two  extremely  small,  two  mode- 
rate, three  intermediate  between  the  two  last 
kinds,  and  one  high,  the  asoents  being  abrupt 
as  before,  the  descents  rather  wavy  than 
stepped).  These  had  all  been  drawn  on  a 
large  scale  with  several  hundred  ordinates, 
and  were  reduced  photographically.  They  form 
an  excellent  practical  illustration  of  the  nature 
of  harmonic  motions. 


6.  The  presumed  different  Characters  of  Keys^  both  Major  and  Minor. 


See  6upr&,  p.  3  loc  to  3 1 1 c.  It  is  first  neces- 
sary to  know  what  is  the  presumed  pheno- 
menon to  account  for.  In  the  discussion  of 
my  paper  'On  the  Measurement  and  Settle- 

«[  ment  of  Musical  Pitch '  (Journal  of  the  Society 
of  Arts,  25  May  1877,  p.  686),  Prof,  (now  Sir 
George)  Maofarren,  Principal  of  the  Boyal 
Academy  of  Music,  spoke  of  *  the  difficulty  of 
representing  the  compositions  of  different  eras, 
which  had  been  written  for  different  standards 
of  pitch,*  and  added  *  it  was  a  marvellous  fact 
that,  while  the  pitch  was  felt  to  be  changed, 
the  impression  of  the  character  of  the  keys 
seemed  to  remain  with  reference  to  the  nominal 
key,  not  to  the  number  of  vibrations  of  each 
particular  note.  Thus  the  key  of  D  at  the 
present  day  represented  the  same  effect  as  was 
produced  by  the  same  key  according  to  one's 
earliest  recollections ;  it  did  not  sound  like  the 
key  of  JSb,  although  it  might  be  of  the  same 
pitch.  If  Mozart*s  symphony  in  C  were  to 
be  played  a  Semitone  lower,  to  bring  it  to  the 
original  pitch,  it  would  not  sound  at  all  the 

^  same.  How  far  this  result  was  subjective  — 
how  much  depended  on  the  imagination  of  the 
hearer,  and  how  much  on  the  physical  facts — 
was  a  deep,  perhaps  an  insoluble  question ;  but  it 
was  one  which  really  ought  to  be  considered.* 
The  Chairman  (Mr.  William  Pole,  FJt.S., 
Mus.  D.  Oxon),  on  the  contrary,  said :  *  In  a  prac- 
tical point  of  view  the  French  did  an  exceed- 
ingly good  thing  when  they  fixed  on  one  pitch, 
.  .  .  and  they  had  practically  done  so,  not 
only  for  France,  but  the  Continent  generally. 
He  had  the  gratification  some  time  ago  of 
hearing  Beethoven's  Sinfonia  Eroica  played 
at  a  Conservatoire  concert  in  ^b,  as  it  should 
be,  but  he  could  not  get  rid  of  the  idea,  when 
he  heard  it  played, at  the  Philharmonic  con- 
certs, that  it  was  in  £q  .* 


The  mention  of  the  performance  of  sym- 
phonies by  Sir  G.  Macfarren  and  Dr.  Pole 
takes  the  whole  question  out  of  the  action  of 
individual  instruments,  in  which  there  is  no 
doubt  of  considerable  variety  depending  on  the 
tonic,  but  this  can  be  traced  in  every  case  to 
some  defect  of  the  instrument  itself,  as  haa 
been  considered  in  the  text  {loc.  cU,).  The 
point  of  the  long  and  short  keys  on  a  piano- 
forte, spoken  of  by  Prof.  Helmholtz,  has 
been  well  worked  out  by  Mr.  G.  Johnstone 
Stoney,  D.Sc.,  F.B.S.  Dublin,  in  a  paper  read 
before  the  Boyal  Dublin  Society  on  16  March 
1883  {Scientific  Proc,  R.  Dublin  Soe.  p.  59), 
who  calls  attention  to  the  fact  that  in  il  all 
the  Fifths  are  on  white  digitals  and  the  major 
Thirds  on  black  digitals,  while  in  ^b  the 
Fifths  are  on  black  and  the  major  Thirds  on 
white  digitals,  and  argues  that  this  must 
make  considerable  difference  in  playing.  Mr. 
Hipkins,  however,  gives  it  as  his  opinion  that 
it  is  impossible  to  tell  in  the  performance  of 
a  first-rate  player  whether  he  is  striking  a 
white  or  a  black  key.  That  would  relegate 
the  difference  to  the  degree  of  skill  of  the 
player.  But  this  does  not  at  all  affect  the 
organ,  the  harmonium,  or  the  voice.  And  by 
reference  to  symphonies  'we  are  constrained 
to  consider  the  question  independently  of  any 
particular  instrument,  as  a  simple  acoustical 
fact. 

In  order  to  ascertain  what  that  fact  is  sup- 
posed to  be,  according  to  recognised  musicians 
of  high  standing,  I  give  a  condensed  re-arrange- 
ment of  the  characters  attributed  to  different 
keys  in  Mr.  Ernst  Pauer's  Elements  of  the 
BeauUfulin  Music  rNovello),  p.  23,  placing 
the  major  and  minor  keys  in  opposite  columns, 
and  proceeding  by  intervals  of  a  Semitone. 


Digitized  by 


Google 


8S0T.  N. 


MISCELLANEOUS  NOTES. 


55t 


Presumed  characters  of 


Major  Keys. 


Minor  Keys, 


C.  Expressive  of  feeling  in  a  pure,  certain, 
and  decisive  manner,  of  innocence,  powerful 
Tesolve,  manly  earnestness,  deep  religious 
feeling. 

08 .  Scarcely  used ;  as  Db  it  has  fulness 
of  tone,  sonorousness,  and  euphony. 

D.  Expressive  of  majesty,  grandeur,  pomp, 
triumph,  festivity,  stateliness. 

Eb»  Greatest  variety  of  expression ;  emi- 
nently masculine,  serious  and  solemn ;  expres- 
sive of  courage  and  determination,  brilHant, 
firm,  dignified. 

JB,  Expressive  of  joy,  magnificence,  splen- 
dour, and  highest  brilliancy;  brightest  and 
znost  powerful  key. 

F,  Expressive  of  peace  and  joy,  also  of 
light  passing  regret  and  religious  sentiment. 

Fz.  Brilliant  and  very  clear;  as  Gd 
expresses  softness  and  richness. 

O,  Favourite  key  of  youth ;  expresses  sin- 
cerity of  faith,  quiet  love,  calm  meditation, 
simple  grace,  pastoral  life,  and  a  certain 
humour  and  brightness. 

Ab.  Full  of  feeling  and  dreamy  expres- 


A,  Full  of  confidence  and  hope,  radiant 
-with  love,  redolent  of  genuine  cheerfulness; 
especially  expresses  sincerity. 


Bb.  Has  an  open,  frank,  clear,  and  bright 
character,  admitting  of  the  expression  of  quiet 
contemplation ;  favourite  classical  key. 

B,  Expresses  boldness  and  pride  in  for- 
tissimo, purity  and  perfect  clearness  in 
pianissimo ;  seldom  used. 


C.  Expressive  of  softness,  longing,  sad- 
ness, earnestness  and  passionate  intensity, 
and  of  the  supernatural. 

Db.  The  most  intensely  melancholy  key. 

D.  Expressive  of  subdued  melancholy, 
grief,  anxiety,  and  solemnity. 

Eb.  Darkest  and  most  sombre  key  of  all ; 
rarely  used. 


E,  Expressive  of  grief,  moumfulness,  and 
restlessness  of  spirit.  mr 

F.  Harrowing,  full  of  melancholy,  at 
times  rising  into  passion. 

F^ .  Dark,  mysterious,  spectral,  and  full 
of  passion. 

O.  Expresses  sometimes  sadness,  at 
others  quiet  and  sedate  joy,  with  gentle  grace 
or  a  slight  touch  of  dreamy  melancholy,  occa- 
sionally rising  to  a  romantic  elevation. 

Ab,  Fit  for  funeral  marches;  full  of  sad, 
heartrending  expression,  as  of  an  oppressed 
and  sorrowing  heart. 

A,  Expresses  tender  womanly  feeling, 
especially  the  quiet  melancholy  sentiment  of 
Northern  nations;  also  fit  for  Boleros  and 
Mauresque  serenades;  and  finally  for  senti- 
ments of  devotion  mingled  with  pious  resigna- 
tion. 

Bb.  Full  of  gloomy  and  sombre  feeling, 
like  Eb ;  seldom  used.  If 

B.  Very  melancholy ;  tells  of  quiet  expecta- 
tion and  patient  hope. 


In  reading  over  this  Table  it  is  impossible 
not  to  feel  that  the  character,  often  contra- 
dictory, arises  from  the  reminiscence  of  pieces 
of  music  in  those  keys,  as  the  author  indeed 
admits  (ib,  p.  22).  Such  a  distinction  as  that 
made  between  Ft  and  Gfb»  which,  in  equal 
temperament,  is  a  mere  matter  of  notation, 
but  is  here  made  to  yield  incompatible  results, 
shews  that  the  writer  was  thinking  more  of 
treatment  than  of  actual  sound.  This  is  con- 
firmed by  his  saying  (t&.  p.  26) :  '  We  shall  often 
find  that  the  general  character  of  a  key  may  be 
changed  by  peculiarities  and  idiosyncrasies  of 
the  composer ;  and  thus  a  key  may  appear  to 
possess  a  cheerful  character  in  the  hands  of 
one  writer,  whilst  another  composer  infuses 
into  it  a  melancholy  expression ;  all  depends 
on  the  treatment,  on  the  individual  feeling  of 
the  composer,  and  on  his  acute  understanding 
of  the  characteristic  qualities  of  the  key  he 
employs.'  The  writer  then  goes  on  to  consider 
the  effect  of  rhythm  and  time,  and  the  different 
characters  which  he  assigns  to  their  varieties, 
independently  of  the  key  employed,  clash  so 
much  with  the  preceding  that  it  is  difficult  to 
know  what  is  supposed  to  belong  to  one  and 
what  to  the  other. 

Now  the  acoustical  facts,  independently  of 
any  particular  instrument  or  temperament  or 
any  errors  of  tuning  or  performance  (both 
numerous  but  variable),  are  these.    Whether 


we  take  just  intonation,  or  that  of  any  uni- 
form linear  or  cyclic  temperament,  carried  on 
to  a  sufficient  number  of  tones  to  prevent  the 
occurrence  of  'wolves'  within  the  piece  of 
music  performed,  the  one  thing  aimed  at  is 
to  have  the  intervals  between  the  same  notes 
of  the  same  scale  precisely  the  same,  at  what- 
ever pitch  they  are  played,  or  however  they 
may  be  conventionally  noted.  If  there  is  any 
difference  between  the  scales  of,  say,  just  A^ 
and  ^'b,  which  have  a  difference  of  70  cents, 
or  equal  A  and  A  b,  which  differ  by  100  cents, 
or  meantone  A  and  Ab^  which  differ  by  76  ^ 
cents,  or  Pythagorean  A  and  A  b,  which  differ 
by  114  cents,  this  difference  must  be  due 
solely  to  pitch.  There  is  no  doubt  that  on 
the  piano,  the  organ,  and  each  instrument  of 
the  orchestra,  the  difference  will  be  consider- 
able and  very  appreciable,  but  that  does  not 
enter  into  oon^eration.  What  effect  does 
simple  difference  of  pitch  in  the  tonic  pro- 
duce ?  In  the  human  voice  and  in  all  instru- 
ments quality  of  tone  varies  together  with 
the  pitch.  A  change  of  tonic  implies  a  dif- 
ferent pitch  for  the  most  frequently  returning 
sounds,  and  those  most  important  to  the 
nature  of  the  key.  Hence  it  produces  a  dif- 
ferent quality  of  tone,  with  a  variation  in  the 
range  of  partials  possessed,  and  consequently 
affects  the  distinctness  of  the  delimitation  of 
the  principal  consonances  and  dissonances  of 


Digitized  by' 


5Sa 


ADDITIONS  BY  THE  TRANSLATOR. 


APP.  XX. 


the  keys,  and  by  that  means  alters  their  audi- 
ble effect.  For  intervals  so  small  as  we  have 
supposed  this  difference  must  necessarily  be 
small,  whereas  the  difference  of  the  keys  of  A 
and  Ab  is  said  to  be  great.  If  so,  it  can  only 
arise  from  errors  of  intonation  or  performance. 
In  the  days  of  the  old  meantone  temperament 
in  its  defective  state  of  12  notes  only  to  the 
Octave,  there  was  a  vast  difference  between 
the  keys  of  A  and  Ab;  the  first  had  all  its 
chords  correct  (supposing  OU  were  not  sharp- 
ened, tnpr«L,  p.  546(2),  the  latter  had  all  the 
chords  involving  Ab  and  Db  (which  had  to  be 
represented  by  OZ  and  CU)  frightfully  erro- 
neous. 

It  seems  to  me  that  the  feeling  of  a  di|- 
ferenoe  in  the  character  of  the  keys  whose 

^  tonics  vary  but  slightly  in  pitch  was  esta- 
blished at  this  time  (in  Sir  George  Mac- 
farren's  younger  days,  b.  1813).  Any  differ- 
ence so  slight  as  a  Semitone  would  have  been 
strongly  felt  (except  in  passing  from  A  ix>  Bbt 
the  two  extreme  good  keys) .  Whereas  for  differ- 
ences of  a  Fifth  there  would  generally  not  have 
been  such  violent  distinctions  (except  at  the  ex- 
tremes Ato  E  and  Bb  to  ^b).  It  would  ap- 
pear that  these  mechanical  distinctions  partly 
influenced  composers  in  their  choice  of  a  key, 
and  produced  what  has  become  an  hereditary 
prejudice,  for  which  there  is  no  longer  any 
ground,  and  which  never  ought  to  have  existed 
in  just  unaccompanied  singing.  But  even  at 
that  time  we  have  composers  ignoring  the 
difference.  Handel  composed  his  dead  march 
in  Saul  in  C,  and  having  written  an  anthem 
('  0  sing  unto  the  Lord  a  new  song  *)  for  St. 

%  James'  Chapel  Koyal,  in  which  the  organ  had 


a  pitch  one  Tone  higher  than  his  own  (sea 
sect.  H.p.  5036,  under  a' 474*1),  he  directed  the 
singers,  as  tiie  voice  parts  were  too  high,  to 
take  them  a  Tone  lower,  and  the  accompany- 
ing organ  to  play  tivo  Tones  lower.  Had 
Handel  any  idea  of  the  innate  di^erence  of 
the  character  of  keys?  and,  if  he  had  not, 
what  does  it  all  amount  to?  Beyond  those 
differences  inevitable  to  varieties  of  pitch, 
already  pointed  out,  and  easily  perceived  by 
slowly  playing  up  the  major  scale  on  any 
instrument  from  its  lowest ,  note,  or  ainging 
it  on  any  voice  from  the  lowest  note  it  can 
reach  easily  (see  p.  544^) — beyond  such  differ- 
ences, all  seems  to  be  subjective,  or  due  to 
hereditary  feeling  created  by  former  defective 
temperaments,  or  at  present  to  mechanical 
errors  of  tuning,  stopping,  or  blowing,  espe- 
cially in  unusual  keys.  Possibly  a  composer 
at  the  present  day  would  write  a  piece  of  a 
totally  different  character,  as  pointed  out  in 
the  table,  according  as  he  made  the  signature 
FZ  OT  Gbf  but  that  must  have  been  a  reaction 
on  his  own  mind,  for  the  tones  he  would  play 
would  be  precisely  the  same  in  both  cases. 

That  tuners  of  the  piano  sometimes  still 
intentionally  tune  unequally,  and  hence  make 
the  effect  of  A  and  A  b  really  very  different,  has 
nothing  whatever  to  do  with  the  matter.  Those 
who  do  so  have  not  learned  their  profession. 
Similarly  for  players  on  a  pianoforte  who 
cannot  equalise  the  effect  of  the  long  and  short 
keys.  But  the  singer  knows  very  well  when  a 
piece  of  music  falls  upon  his  bad  notes,  and 
ruthlessly  transposes  the  key,  quite  reckless 
of  these  presumed  varieties  of  key-character. 


7.  Dr.  W.  H.  Stone's  Bestoration  of  i6'foot  C,  to  the  Orchestra. 


See  p.  175c  on  the  deepest  tones  which  can 
be  heard.  The  following  is  condensed  from 
information  furnished  me  by  Br.  Stone. 

Dr.  Stone  has  for  some  years  endeavoured  to 
restore  to  the  orchestra  the  lower  notes  of  the 
16-foot  Octave,  which  appear  to  have  been 
neglected  of  late.  It  seems  to  him  a  contra- 
diction that,  while  the  organ  possessed  that 
Octave,  and  another,  the  32-foot,  below  it,  and 
while  even  an  instrument  of  so  comparatively 
feeble  a  tone  as  the  pianoforte  could  obtain 
these  deep  notes,  they  should  be  absent  from 
the  full  band.  Most  of  the  great  composers 
have  employed  them,  especially  Beethoven  and 
%  Onslow.  Many  passages  of  their  compositions 
had  to  be  partially  transposed,  often  (as  in  the 
C  minor  Symphony  of  Beethoven)  much  to  the 
detriment  of  the  general  effect.  In  the  Trio 
of  this  great  work  a  scale  passage  occurs 
several  times  for  the  double  basses  alone, 
beginning  on  the  16-foot  G^  But  this  note 
being  entirely  absent  on  the  ordinary  three- 
string  basses,  as  used  in  England,  it  was  there 
customary  to  take  it  either  ^together  or  in 
part  in  the  Octave  above.  Some  players, 
indeed,  were  in  the  habit  of  letting  down  the 
A,  or  lowest  string,  by  a  Tone  to  O,  for  this 
special  passage ;  but  the  resonance  of  a  string 
thus  slackened  was  far  inferior  to  what  could 
be  obtained  by  more  legitimate  means.  The 
fine  part  for  the  contrafagotto  in  the  same 
symphony,  descending  to  C,,  was  usually 
omitted,  or  played  an  Octave  higher  by  the 
ophicleide.    The  Pastoral  Symphony  likewise 


frequently  contains  F,  natural,  a  note  quite 
unattainable  except  on  the  four-string  basses, 
whose  lowest  note  is  E,  (p.  i8c). 

It  was  obvious,  in  attempting  to  remedy 
this  defect,  that  of  the  three  modes  by  which 
vibrations  in  a  stretched  string  may  be 
slackened,  two,  namely,  length  and  thidkness, 
were  inadmissible.  The  first  renders  the  m- 
strument  so  large  as  to  be  unwieldy  and  out 
of  the  reach  of  an  ordinary  arm.  The  second 
was  found  to  cause  rotation  of  the  string  under 
the  impulse  of  the  bow  acting  at  its  periphery, 
and  thus  to  generate  false  notes.  The  third 
remained,  in  increasing  the  specific  gravity  of 
the  string  without  enlarging  its  diameter.  This 
was  satisfactorily  accomplished  by  covering  a 
gut  string  with  heavy  copper  wire  saoh  as  is 
used  for  the  lowest  strings  of  pianos.  The 
note  C,  was  obtained,  and  an  instrument  thus 
strung  was  exhibited  in  London  in  the  Inter- 
national Exhibition  of  1872. 

But  it  became  clear  that  to  give  the  new 
notes  full  power,  and  to  prevent  the  danger  of 
shaking  the  instrument  to  pieces,  a  means  of 
strengthening  the  belly  in  the  direction  of 
strain  was  required,  which  should  not  unduly 
increase  the  weight  of  the  sound-board.  This 
requisite  was  ingeniously  fulfilled  by  Mr. 
Meeson  :  -  -  Four  strips  of  white  pine  are  glued 
on  to  the  back  of  the  belly,  running  its  whole 
length,  one  on  one  side  of  the  ordinary  bass- 
bar,  and  three  on  its  other  side,  thus  eoire- 
sponding  in  number,  and  to  a  certain  degree 
in  position,  to  the  increased  number  of  strings* 

Digitized  by  VjOOQlC 


BEOT.  N. 


MISCELLANEOUS  NOTES. 


553 


Two  of  them  cross  and  intercept  the  usual 
/-shaped  sound-holes,  thus  removing  a  weak 
place  in  the  belly,  and  causing  it'  to  vibrate 
more  homogeneously.  They  appeared  on  trial 
to  add  great  power  to  the  instrument  through- 
out, and  to  remove  the  inequality  and  varying 
intensity  of  vibration  which  exists  on  most 
old  instruments  even  by  celebrated  makers, 
and  which  musicians  usually  designate  by 
the  term  •  wolf.'  The  bars  are  curved  to  an 
olliptical  shape  to  fit  the  hollow  of  the  belly, 
and  to  give  the  greatest  resistance  to  com- 
pression with  the  smallest  quantity  of  mate- 
rial. Even  in  a  double  bass  the  quantity  of 
wood  required  is  very  small,  and  from  its 
lightness  when  perfectly  dry  it  hardly  exceeds 
an  ounce  in  weight.  From  their  shape  and 
function  they  are  termed  eUiptical  terision 
bars. 

It  appeared  from  subsequent  experiments 
that  the  same  system  was  applicable  to  the 
smaller  members  of  the  viol  and  violin  family, 
giving  an  increased  sonority  and  firmness  to 
the  tone.    It  succeeds  best  with  violins  of 


sweet  but  feeble  quality,  and  in  some  of  the 
older  Itijian  instruments,  where  the  progress 
of  decay  had  to  a  certain  extent  diminished 
the  volume  of  sound. 

The  contrafagotto  or  double  bassoon  as 
made  on  Dr.  Stone's  designs  by  Herr  Haseneier 
of  Coblenz,  consists  of  a  tube  i6  feet  4  inches 
long,  truly  conical  in  its  bore,  and  enlarging 
from  j^-inch  diameter  at  the  reed  to  4  inches  at 
the  bell  or  lower  extremity.  It  is  curved  on 
itself  for  convenience  of  manipulation,  so 
that  in  actual  length  it  is  ,about  equal  to  the 
ordinary  bassoon.  Its  extreme  compass  is 
from  C,  to  c',  but  its  ordinary  range  is  to  g 
only,  the  other  notes  being  difficult  to  bring 
out.  Haydn  gives  a  part  to  such  an  instru- 
ment in  his  Creation^  Mozart  uses  it  occa- 
sionally, Beethoven  frequently,  Mendelssohn 
sometimes.  ^ 

Both  of  these  instruments  I  have  heard  in 
use.  Their  tone  is  not  perfectly  continuous, 
but  is  very  good,  and  when  played  in  conjunc- 
tion with  other  instruments,  musically  effec- 
tive. 


8.  On  the  Action  of  Reeds. 


(See  pp.  95  to  100.)  Knowing  the  long, 
patient,  and  practical  attention  which  Mr. 
Hermann  Smith  had  paid  to  the  action  of 
reeds,  I  requested  him  to  furnish  me  with  an 
account  of  the  results  of  his  experience.  He 
obligingly  sent  me  a  series  of  elaborate  and 
extensive  notes,  which  the  space  at  my  com- 
mand utterly  precludes  me  from  giving  at 
proper  length,  and  which  I  am  therefore  forced 
to  represent  by  the  meagrest  possible  outline, 
with  the  hope  that  they  will  appear  elsewhere 
in  suitable  detail.  Only  passages  in  inverted 
commas  contain  Mr.  Hermann  Smith's  actual 
words,  the  rest  is  my  own  necessarily  imperfect 
attempt  to  condense  his  statements. 

Beeds  maybe  classed  as,  i.  single^  whether 
stHking  (in  clarinet  of  cane,  and  reed-pipe  of 
organ  of  metal)  or  free  (harmonium  and 
American  organ,  both  of  metal);  2.  paired 
(bassoon  and  oboe,  both  of  cane,  in  action 
compressible;  horns  and  larynx,  both  mem- 
branous, in  action  extensible) ;  3.  streaming 
(flutes,  flageolets,  flue-pipes  of  organ,  all  of 
rushing  air,  in  action  abstracting).  The  last 
kind  has  been  partly  considered,  supr&, 
pp.  396-7. 

In  the  clarinet  the  reed  is  straight  and 
very  thin  at  the  tip,  but  the  edge  of  the  sec- 
tion of  the  wooden  tube  against  which  it 
strikes  presents  a  slight  curve,  whereas  in  the 
organ  pipe  the  reed  is  curved  and  the  edge 
against  which  it  strikes  is  straight  (p.  9^c). 
The  time  of  vibration  consists  (i)  of  the  time 
of  forward  motion,  which  may  vary  slightly ; 

(2)  of  the  time  of  rest,  which  may  vary  greatly  ; 

(3)  of  the  time  of  recoil,  which  does  not  vary 
*  When  the  reed  is  placed  in  the  mouth,  the 
air  on  both  surfaces  of  the  reed  is  of  equal 
pressure,  and  on  increase  of  strength  in  the 
wind,  the  tendency  would  be  to  separate 
the  reed  still  further  from  the  edge  of  the 
mouthpiece,  were  it  not  that  a  current  of  air 
quickly  passing  into  a  tube  exercises  suction 
at  the  orifice  of  entry;  therefore  the  elastic 
reed  yields  in  the  direction  of  the  place  of 
suction,  so  that  it  is  held  there.  The  current 
of  wind  having  been  sent  forward  with  im- 


petus, leaves  behind  it  a  partial  vacuum,  which 
is  strongest  close  upon  the  inner  face  of  the 
tongue.  There,  then,  is  a  region  of  least 
pressure,  which  continues  to  exist  during  the 
transit  of  the  pulse  of  compressed  air  to  the 
first  found  point  of  outlet.  When  that  point  is 
reached,  the  external  air  rushes  in  and  restores 
equilibrium,  and  in  doing  so  causes  the  shock 
of  arrested  motion  in  which  the  reed  recoils, 
and  forthwith  the  action  commences  as  before.'  f 

The  clarinet  should  not  be  described  as  a 
stopped  pipe,  though  both  have  unevenly  num- 
bered partials  and  give  similar  pitch  for  simi- 
lar length,  because  *  in  the  clarinet  there  is  a 
propulsive  current  going  through  the  pipe ;  in 
the  stopped  pipe,  on  the  contrary,  there  is  an 
abstracting  current  acting  outside  by  suction.' 
Too  much  has  been  attributed  to  the  cylin- 
drical bore  for  producing  only  the  unevenly 
numbered  partials.  *  An  oboe  reed  fixed 
on  the  clarinet  tube  gives  oboe  pitch  of  tone 
and  oboe  partials.'  The  Japanese  Hichi-riJci 
has  an  inverted  conical  bore,  that  is,  the  dia- 
meter is  the  smallest  at  the  point  furthest 
from  the  reed.  *  Like  the  clarinet,  it  g^ves 
notes  which  are  an  Octave  lower  in  pitch 
than  would  be  calculated  from  its  length. 
The  first  note  after  the  fundamental  is  the 
Twelfth.  The  reed  is  broad,  not  single  as  on  ^ 
the  clarinet,  but  double  and  as  the  bassoon 
reed,  differing,  however,  in  having  an  enlarged 
base  where  it  fixed  into  the  tube.  ...  On 
substituting  an  oboe  reed,  the  pitches  of  the 
notes  correspond  to  those  of  the  oboe  and  the 
first  tone  after  is  the  Octave.  ...  If  the  end 
of  the  pipe  is  placed  full  within  the  mouth, 
and  is  blown  tl^ough  without  the  use  of  any 
reed  whatever  (and  without  any  action  from 
the  lips),  clear  and  powerful  sounds  are  elicited, 
varying  as  the  openings  of  the  holes  are  varied, 
provided  one  of  the  upper  holes  is  left  open 
...  it  is  indifferent  whether  the  end  of  the 
wide  diameter  or  that  of  the  narrow  is  taken 
into  the  mouth,  either  way  sounds  are  in  this 
manner  readily  produced.'  This  effect  Mr. 
Hermann  Smith  attributed  to  a  stream  reed 
from  the  open  hole. 

Digitized  by  V^OOQ IC 


554 


ADDITIONS  BY  THE  TRANSLATOB. 


AW.  XX, 


Bassoons  and  oboes  have  paired  reeds,  \7hich 
touch  down  their  oater  edge,  and  do  not  vibrate 
length-wise  but  cross-wise,  so  that  a  transverse 
section  through  them  has  alternately  the  form 
of  the  outer  lines  in  fig.  63,  p.  387c,  when 
they  are  open,  and  of  two  parallel  lines  when 
they  are  dosed.  The  reeds  are  sections  from 
a  small  hollow  reed-plant  of  particular  growth 
{arundo  donax  or  saHva)^  niade  very  min  at 
the  tip,  and  rendered  supple  by  the  moisture 
of  the  mouth.  The  player's  lip  restricts  the 
size  of  the  oval  in  notes  of  high  pitch.  *  The 
pressure  of  wind  would  keep  the  pair  of  reeds 
apart  but  for  the  influence  of  the  suction  when 
the  current  is  thrown  through.  The  vibration 
therefore  is  produced  by  the  same  kind  of  action 
as  in  the  clarinet,  but  there  is  a  new  mechani- 
cal method  for  bringing  it  about.' 

^  The  membranous  reeds  formed  by  the  lips 
and  vocal  chords  are  reeds  of  extension^  be- 
ginning to  vibrate  from  a  state  of  closurej 
contrary  to  all  other  reeds.  In  horns  the  cup 
acts  as  an  exhaust  chamber,  and  when  it  is 
too  large  the  upper  notes  cannot  be  well  pro- 
duced ;  *  that  is  to  say,  the  necessary  degree  of 
vacuum  cannot  be  brought  about  in  time  to 
coincide  with  the  reciprocating  return  of  the 
column  of  air  in  the  tube.'  In  the  larynx 
the  ventricles  of  Morgagni  between  the  true  and 
false  chords  probably  act  as  exhaust  chambers. 
The  stream  reeds  have  been  already  con- 
sidered (p.  396c'),  but  Mr.  Hermann  Smith 
has  developed  his  theory  of  displacement 
actio7i,  or  the  actual  tone  of  air  under  cleav- 
age, deduced  originally  from  observations  of 
the  different  sounds  of  wind  sweeping  through 
the  branches  of  leafless  trees,  in  which  tone  is 

5f  produced  without  a  vibrating  agent.  Mr.  Her- 
mann Smith  found  the  common  doctrine  of 
friction  unsatisfactory.  In  1870  he  had  made 
a  series  of  rods  about  5  ft.  long,  the  sides  of 
each  having  same  smoothness  and  of  the  uni- 
form width  of  i^  inches,  with  a  V-Bhaped  or 
triangular  section,  and  these  he  swept  swiftly 
through  the  air  like  swords,  sharp  edge  first. 
He  found  that,  although  the  friction  surface 
was  similar  on  each,  they  developed  different 
notes,  which  he  discovered  to  be  according  to 
the  thickness  of  the  back,  the  pitches  being 
inversely  as  the  thickness.  C  528  vib.  re- 
quires a  thickness  somewhat  less  than  half  an 
inch.  *  Covering  irregularly  the  tips  of  the  rods 
did  not  affect  the  sound.  Half  an  inch  thick- 
ness of  spongy  felt  fixed  on  the  back  of  the 
rod,  the  same  width  being  preserved,  lowered 
the  pitch  a  Fourth.    The  felt  entangled  air  in 

If  its  pores,  so  that  the  vacuum  by  suction  was 
less  perfect. . . .  With  less  speed  of  stroke  the 
pitch  is  again  lowered. ...  In  organ  pipes,  &c., 
in  all  wind  instruments,  a  certain  displacement 
of  air  must  take  place  immediately  near  the 
agent  of  vibration,  in  which  space  a  right  de- 
gree of  vacuum  is  requisite,  else  the  right  note 
will  not  follow.  To  this  result  all  the  devices 
of  mouthpieces  tend.  This  work  of  displace- 
ment in  the  origination  of  sound  raises  a  ques- 
tion distinct  from  the  transmission  of  sounds 
in  waves.' 

The  free  reed  is  supposed  to  have  been 
adopted  from  China.  But  the  European  and 
Chinese  forms  are  different.  The  Chinese 
reed  is  stamped  out  in  the  same  piece  as  the 
frame,  with  which  it  lies  level.  The  reeds  act 
upon  tubes  which  (agreeing  with  M.  W.  Weber's 
law,  though  made  long  before  its  discovery) 


are  three-quarters  of  the  half -wave  length. 
The  harmonium  reed  is  placed  above  the  frame, 
and  the  end  turns  up  from  it.  If  it  is  set  level 
with  the  frame  it  will  not  vibrate.  To  produce 
vibration  a  stream  of  air  must  pass  between 
the  tongue  and  the  frame,  producing  a  partial 
vacuum  on  the  underside  of  the  reed,  and  the 
amount  of  suction  thus  caused  must  be  pro- 
perly graduated  or  there  will  be  no  action. 
The  chief  peculiarity  of  the  free  reed  is  that 
the  pitch  is  only  slightly  affected  by  the 
cavities  with  which  it  is  associated,  but  these 
boxes  or  cavities,  according  to  their  dinaensions, 
and  governed  by  the  operation  of  partial  ocdn- 
slon,  mainly  determine  the  qttaUty  of  tone. 

The  reed  is  not  properly  compared  to  a 
vibrating  rod,  because  the  reed  actually  in  use 
is  not  uniform.  In  a  series,  the  low  reeds 
are  thickest  at  tip  and  thinnest  at  root; 
high  reeds  thinnest  at  tip  and  thickest  at 
root.  Hence  they  are  affected  differently  by 
different  pressures  of  wind,  an^  alter  their 
pitch  differently.  Expressive  playing  there- 
fore becomes  playing  out  of  tune.  Only  with 
a  constant  blast  will  a  free  reed  maintain  a 
constant  pitch.  The  stronger  blast  flattens 
deep  reeds  and  sharpens  high  ones,  and  from 
this  cause  arises  much  of  the  painful  disso- 
nance of  series  of  chords  played  on  these  in- 
struments. 

To  remedy  this  defect  the  action  of  the 
wind  on  the  tongue  in  the  American  organ  is 
limited  by  making  the  frame  very  thin,  which 
is  *  dished  out '  underneath  till  the  edge  passed 
by  the  tongue  is  barely  thicker  than  the  tongue, 
instead  of  being  8  or  10  times  as  thick  as 
on  the  harmonium.  The  suction  in  the  har- 
monium is  longer  in  time  and  stronger  in  de- 
gree, the  tongue  moves  a  greater  distance,  and 
more  intensity  of  tone  is  produced.  *The 
American  reed  cannot  make  a  deep  excursion, 
for  the  suction  is  spent  as  soon  as  the  tongue 
gets  below  the  edge  of  the  frame.'  It  is  there- 
fore not  suited  for  expression,  but  produces  a 
smooth  and  flexible  kmd  of  tone. 

Mr.  Hermann  Smith  considers  the  harsh- 
ness of  the  free  reed,  resulting  from  its  large 
number  of  partials,  to  be  chiefly  due  to  its  pro- 
portions. *  The  tongue  is  inordinately  long  in 
proportion  to  its  width,  and  hence  under  the 
stress  of  the  wind  (which  necessarily  shifts  its 
incidence  during  the  movements  of  the  tongue 
and  its  re-course)  there  is  developed  a  diagonal 
strain  or  torsion*  from  one  comer  at  the  tip  of 
the  tongue  to  the  opposite  comer  of  the  root, 
so  that  a  lateral  irregular  motion  is  set  up 
accompanying  the  longitudinal  vibration.' 
Hence  he  concludes  that  *  the  long  reed  is  a 
wrong  reed,'  and  that,  *  in  view  of  its  liability 
to  lateral  torsion,  the  rectangular  form  is  about 
the  worst. . . .  Trial  of  various  reeds  shews  that 
a  long  rectangular  reed  is  strident  in  tone,  that 
increase  of  width  in  reeds  brings  in  increasing 
proportion  smoothness  of  tone,  and  that  the 
width  may  be  increased  till  it  equals  the 
length.' 

In  *  voicing,'  a  bend  is  made  across  the 
tongue,  turning  the  point  upwards.  This  some- 
what checks  lateral  vibration.  By  an  early 
plan  of  his,  *  reeds  were  yoked  toother  by  a 
bar  across  the  middle  of  the  lengUi,  and  the 
improved  quality  of  tone  was  obviously  doe  to 
the  fa6t  that  the  two  reeds  were  equivalent  to 
one  broad  reed,  and  that  the  bar  across  hin- 
dered the  operation  of  anvdiagoxu^  strain  or 
Digitized  by  VjDOv  LC 


SECT.  K.. 


MISCELLANEOUS  NOTES. 


555 


twist  during  vibration. . . .  When  a  reed  is  much 
ourved  it  is  slow  in  speech,  and  a  great  amount 
of  wind  passes  wastefully,  compensated  only 
by  the  smoother  tone.'  Mr.  Hermann  Smith 
says  his  *  best  toned  reeds  have  been  series  in 
which,  according  to  his  design,  the  openings 
made  by  the  curve  given  to  the  reed  were  filled 
up  at  the  sides  by  arched  blocks  added  to  the 
top  of  the  frame,  following  the  line  of  curve. 
The  discontinuity  was  therefore  sharply  de- 
fined, yet  the  tones  were  mellow  and  rich.  A 
reed  mounted  on  wood  surface  may  have  its 
quality  greatly  changed  by  an  interposed  pad 
of  leather  or  felt  between  the  reed  frame  and 
the  wood,  the  extreme  harshness  disappears 
and  the  tone  is  smoother  altogether,  shewing 
how  much  that  is  unpleasant  is  due  to  the 
jarring  from  arrested  motion.* 

Mr.  Hermann  Smith's  *  conclusions  are  that, 
in  the  making  of  free  reed  instruments,  broad 
reeds  should  be  used,  and  with  broad  channels 
or  boxes  or  cavities  of  varied  shape ;  that  within 
the  large  chamber  small  suction  chambers 
should  be  placed  below  or  beyond  the  reed 
tongue,  in  imitation  of  the  ventricles  of  the 
larynx ;  and  if  these  afford  areas  and  cavities 
suitable  for  the  displacement  accordant  with 
the  pitches,  the  speech  will  be  quickened  and 
firmness  given  to  the  tones,  or,  in  other  words, 
the  mechanical  motion  of  air  and  reed  will  be 
steadied.  The  rectangular  form  of  reed,  except 
when  stops  of  hard  metallic  quality  are  re- 
quired, should  be  abandoned,  and  broad  shapes 
substituted,  having  tips  semicircular,  semi-oval, 
or  ovate,  or  shapes  to  ensure  a  central  line  of 
strain.  Weighting  the  tips  of  reeds  should  be 
avoided  as  much  as  possible.* 

Mr.  Hermann  Smith's  latest  device  in  the 
treatment  of  reeds  is  designed  to  overcome  the 
difficulties  of  inordinately  long  or  weighted 
reeds.  His  plan  is  '  to  use  metal  or  material  of 


uniform  thickness,  and  to  get  the  degrees  of 
flattening  by  drilling  out  or  excising  portions 
of  material  at  or  near  the  root  of  the  reed, 
and  then  to  fill  such  spaces  as  are  thus  made 
with  other  fixed  pieces  of  metal  that  are  neu- 
tral, and  do  not  enter  into  vibration.  Thus 
the  sides  of  the  reed  tongues  remain  with  the 
fibre  intact,  unweakened  by  thinning  or  scrap- 
ing, which  takes  the  best  vigour  out  of  the 
reed.  Any  degree  of  fiattening  may  be  at- 
tained according  as  the  excision  is  made  to 
extend  up  the  tongue.  A  like  treatment  of  the 
half  of  the  tongue  forming  the  tip  will  pro- 
duce the  opposite  effect,  sharpening  pitch  by 
lightening  the  tip ;  the  spaces  left  by  the  ex- 
cised portions  are  then  covered  with  lighter 
material,  such  as  goldbeater's  skin.' 

Mr.  Hermann  Smith  states  that  his  device  or 
of  *  adding  to  the  large  cavities  small  exhaust 
chambers  or  cup-like  cavities,  fixed  just  below 
the  tongue  of  the  reed,  causes  the  most  un- 
manageable reeds,  even  those  in  the  32-foot 
Octave,  when  made  broad  on  the  above  plan, 
to  render  good  musical  service,  to  be  free  in 
speech,  and  to  produce  a  full  pervading  quality 
of  tone,  devoid  of  the  harshness  of  long  reeds 
having  heavily  weighted  tips.'  Mr.  Augustus 
Stroh  (see  Sect.  M.  No.  2,  p.  542^)  informs  me 
that  he  has  been  led  to  a  similar  contrivance 
in  a  machine  he  has  recently  constructed. 

For  further  details  of  Mr.  Hermann  Smith's 
inventions  respecting  reeds,  see  Specifications 
to  his  Patents  1878,  No.  227  and  No.  4942 ; 
1880,  No.  68 ;  and  1884,  No.  7777.  A  large 
amount  of  varied  information  may  also  be 
found  in  his  treatise  entitled '  In  the  Organ  and 
in  the  Orchestra,*  now  (1884-.5)  publi^ing  in  ^, 
Musical  Opinion^  a  monthly  magazine.  Of  this 
treatise  25  chapters  have  already  appeared  full 
of  interesting  elucidations  of  instrumental  diffi- 
culties. 


9.  Postscript, 


Standard  Musical  Pitch  in  England.— la 
consequence  of  a  communication  from  our 
Foreign  Office,  due  to  the  Belgian  change  of 
pitch  (p.  $oid)t  Sir  G.  A.  Macfarren,  Prin- 
cipal of  the  Boyal  Academy  of  Music,  con- 
vened a  public  meeting  of  musicians,  theorists, 
instrument-makers,  and  their  friends,  *  to  con- 
sider the  desirability  of  a  standard  musical 
pitch  for  the  United  Kingdom.'  It  took  place 
on  20  June  1885,  and  was  well  attended. 
Three  resolutions  were  passed:  (i)  declaring 
uniformity  desirable:  (2)  recommending  French 
pitch  ;  (3)  declaring  it  advisable  to  teSae  steps 
for  its  adoption  in  civil  and  military  bands. 
A  committee  of  4  theorists,  15  musicians,  and 
4  instrument-makers  was  appointed  to  carry 
out  the  resolutions.  See  the  Times^  22  June 
1885,  p.  7,  col.  2,  and  p.  9,  col.  3,  and  Musical 
Opinion,  i  July  1885,  p.  493. 

Addenda  to  History  of  Pitch.  In  1845  t^e 
pitch  of  the  Philharmonic  Society  was  0^447*1, 
according  to  a  fork  tuned  in  the  orchestra  at 
that  date  by  Mr.  B.  S.  Bockstro. 

Herr  Eduard  Strauss,  of  Vienna,  perform- 
ing at  the  Inventions  Exhibition  1885,  used 
a'  452*5,  but  the  bandsmen  said  the  pitch  of 
the  opera  was  nearly  a  Quartertone  fiatter,  say 
a' 447. 

The  band  of  the  Pomeranian  (Blucher) 
Hussars  performing  at  the  same  Exhibition 
used  a'  460-8,  or,  as  the  bandmaster  said, 


'exactly  a   Semitone   sharper   than   French 
pitch.' 

Effect  of  Rust,  dc,  on  Tuning-forks.  See 
p.  445  d\  Mr.  Bockstiro  possesses  a  fork  which 
in  1859  was  at  French  pitch,  and  now  through 
rust  (and  possibly  bad  treatment)  shews  only 
a'  424-5,  that  is,  has  gone  down  by  42  cents, 
far  exceeding  any  other  fork  examined. 

Flute  Intonation.  Mr.  B.  S.  Bockstro,  in 
June  1885,  kindly  brought  me  an  eight-keyed  «r 
flute,  about  40  years  old,  an  excellent  instru-  ^' 
ment  of  its  kind.  This  he  played  so  as  to 
preserve  its  natural  intonation  without  cor- 
rection. Before  striking  any  new  note  it  was 
ascertained  that  g"  404  vib.  remained  constant. 
The  a'  461  vib.  was  a  quarter  of  a  Tone  too 
sharp.  The  result  gave  in  cents,  reckoning  g* 
as  1200-0,  c' 488,  c'5564,  d'  678,  d'J763,  «' 
906,  /'  1000,  f%  1098,  ^  1200  =  0,  ^Z  92,  a' 
229,  a%  292, 6'43i,  c"  513,  c"5  566.  This  seems 
meant  for  meantone  intonation,  sharpening 
many  of  the  sharps  to  pass  as  the  flats  above 
them.  But  even  in  this  case  the  intonation 
was  imperfect,  and  Mr.  Bockstro  thought  it 
was  rather  due  to  a  series  of  compromises. 

Mr.  B.  S.  Bockstro  brought  at  the  same 
time  the  '  Bockstro  model '  flute,  invented  by 
himself,  to  have  a  more  correct  equal  intona- 
tion than  Boehm's.  This  was  blown  in  the 
same  way,  but  in  this  case  a'  was  always 
brought  to  452  vib.    Besult  in  cents,  reckon. 


Digitized  by  V^OOQ IC 


556 


ADDITIONS  BY  THE  TRANSLATOR. 


APP.  XX. 


ing  a*  as  i200«o,  was  d  312,  c'«4o6,  d'  506, 
d'ft  601,  «'  697,/ 801, /JJ  895,  g'  looi,  gr'5  1097, 
o'  i200;so,  a%  102,  2/  201,  c"  304,  c"5  404. 
This  is  very  good,  and  may  be  better  than 
the  above  numbers  shew,  as,  on  account  of  the 
difficulty  of  sustaining  notes  on  the  flute 
without  variation,  it  was  not  possible  to  deter- 
mine the  pitch  of  each  note  within  less  than 
I  vib.  in  a  second.  With  regard  to  the  lowest 
d  from  the  open  end  of  the  flute,  Mr.  Bockstro 
says  he  leaves  it  purposely  too  sharp  in  relation 
to  a',  because  it  is  easy,  by  management  of 
lip,  to  blow  it  in  tune,  but  if  it  were  originally 
in  tune,  it  would  not  sound  sharp  enough  in 
very  soft  passages. 

Siamese  Scales.  The  King  of  Siam  sent 
over  his  Court  Band  with  their  instruments  to 
^  the  London  Inventions  Ejthibition  1885,  and 
the  Siamese  minister  obligingly  allowed  Mr. 
Hipkins  and  myself  to  determine  the  musical 
scale.  Prince  Prisdang  told  us  that  the  in- 
tention was  to  divide  the  Octave  into  7  equal 
intervals,  each  of  which  would  then  have 
1 7 1  -43  cents.  Hence  the  following  comparison. 
The  scales  are  given  as  usual  in  cents  from  the 
lowest  note. 

Theoretical  scale : — o,  171,  343,  514,  686, 
857,  1029,  1200  cents,  having  a  neutral  Second 
171  lying  between  100  and  200,  a  neutral  Third 
343  lying  between  3cx>  and  400,  a  slightly 
sharpened  Fourth  514  for  500,  a  slightly  flat- 
tened Fifth  686  for  700,  a  neutral  Sixth  857 
lying  between  800  and  9CX),  and  a  neutral 
Seventh  1029  lying  between  1000  and  iioo, 
but  much  nearer  the  former.  As  there  is  no 
harmonic  interval  but  the  Octave,  and  as  the 
^  Siamese  seem  to  tune  by  Octaves  and  single 
degrees,  there  is  room  for  much  variation  from 
the  ideal  intonation,  as  shewn  in  the  following 
observed  scales. 

Banat  ek  or  wood  harmonicon,  first  Octave 
o,  208,  326,  537,  698,  883,  1048,  1208,  second 
Octave  (pitch  of  lowest  note  382-6  vib.),  o,  200, 
359.537,  7".  883,  1057,  1222,  third  (incom- 
plete)  Octave  o,  193,  347,  549,  698  (two  more 
bars,  too  high  to  measure).  This  instrument 
is  tuned  by  lumps  of  wax  mixed  with  some 
heavy  substance  stuck  to  the  underpart  of  the 
bar.  The  tuning  lump  having  fallen  from  the 
second  bar  of  the  first  two  Octaves,  it  was 
quite  out  of  tune,  and  its  proper  pitch  (regis- 
tered above)  was  determined  by  a  comparison 
with  other  instruments.  In  the  Banat,  p.  518, 
No.  85,  all  the  lumps  had  been  removed, 
hence,  it  was  entirely  out  of  tune, 
f  Banat  t'hong  or  brass  harmonicon  (pitch 
of  lowest  note  382*6  vib.)  scale  o,  200,  340, 
537i  699,  881,  1043,  1207. 

Banat  lek  or  steel  harmonicon,  first  Oc- 
tave (the  second  bar  absent),  o,  327,  519,  679, 
856,  1075,  1202,  second  Octave  (pitch  of  low- 
est note,  385-5  vib.),  o,  150,  299,  447,  614,  743, 
960,  1 1 79,  third  (incomplete)  Octave  o,  90, 
222,  430,  609. 

Tak*hay  or  crocodile,  a  three-stringed  in- 
strument with  high  frets,  played  with  a  conical 
plectrum,  o,  198, 362,  528, 720,  890, 1080,  1250. 

Hence  52  single  degrees  were  examined, 
each  of  which  should  have  had  theoretically 
171*43  cents.  In  reality  5  were  less  than  132, 
8  between  140  and  159,  12  between  160  and 
167,  9  between  170  and  179,  3  between  180 
and  185,  6  between  190  and  198,  and  9  be- 
tween 2CO  and  219.  Hence  only  15  approached 


to  equal  Tones,  and  only  2  approached  to  eqnal 
Semitones,  both  sets  being  clearly  erroneons, 
while  the  21  between  160  and  179  were  toler- 
ably close  approximations  to  the  ideal.  Bear- 
ing these  variations  in  mind,  it  is  probable 
that  p.  518,  Nos.  81, 82,  and  83,  at  least,  be- 
longed to  this  system  of  7  intentionally  equal 
heptatones,  as  they  may  be  called.  And  this 
confirms  the  conception  that  Salendro,  p.  518, 
Nos.  94  and  95,  consists  ideally  of  5  equal 
pentatones. 

The  instruments  were  beautifully  and 
artistically  ornamented,  the  execution  by  the 
musicians  was  florid  and  musicianly  in  aoca- 
rate  and  varied  rhythm,  there  was  an  obser- 
vance of  light  and  shade,  together  with  a  clear 
conception  of  melody,  but  none  of  harmony. 
Besides  the  harmonicons  there  were  kettles  or 
gongs  (k'hong),  a  three-stringed  viol  (saw  t'hai), 
a  two-stringed  fiddle  (saw  Chine),  the  three- 
stringed  crocodile  (tak'hay) ;  reed  instruments, 
flutes,  and  drums. 

Japayiese  Scales,  see  pp.  519  and  522,  Nos. 
no  to  139.  In  July  1885,  Mr.  Isawa,  Director 
of  the  Musical  Institute,  Tokio,  Japan,  sent  to 
the  Inventions  Exhibition  several  tuning-forks 
and  tables.  From  the  tables  it  appeared  that 
the  classical  12  Bitsu  or  Semitones  resulted 
from  tuning  1 1  perfect  Fifths  up  (or  Fourths 
down),  and  then  a  Fifth  too  flat  by  a  Pytha- 
gorean comma,  giving  the  scale  :  o,  1 14,  204, 
318,  408,  522,  612,  702,  816,  906,  1020,  mo, 
1200  cents.  But  the  13  forks  sent  had  the 
following  pitch  (as  determined  by  the  Trans- 
lator), the  number  and  name  of  the  Bitsu  and 
the  name  of  the  nearest  European  note  at 
French  pitch  being  prefixed :-  -I.  Ichikotsa 
d'  2927,  n.  Tangin  d'n  305*6,  III.  Hiyojo 
d  326*2,  IV.  ShOretsu  /  343*  i,  V.  Shimoma 
fn  3657.  VI.  Sojo g'  391*5,  VII. Fusho  ^I 4io-ii 
VIII.  Waushiki  a'  437,  IX.  Bankei  a|  460, 
X.  Banshiki  6'  491-5,  XI.  Shinsen  d'  S^7'3^ 
XII.  Kamimu  d'Z  549'5i  I'.  Ichikotsu  d"  585*4, 
this  gives  the  scale  in  cents :  o,  75,  188,  275, 
385.  503.  583.  693»  782,897,  986,  1091,  1200. 

Mr.  Isawa  also  sent  forks  for  tuning  the 
popular  scale  Hiradioshi  (p.  519a,  Nos.  no  to 
112)  in  two  forms,  old  and  new,  both  different 
from  those  already  given.  Old  style  in  cents 
o,  102,  502,  706,  809,  1x97,  evidently  meant 
for  just  o,  112,  498,  702,  814,  1200.  Nev 
style  in  cents  o,  85,  502,  708,  793,  1200,  for 
Pythagorean  o,  90,  498,  702,  792,  1200. 

Mr.  Isawa  also  sent  a  Standard  Tuning- 
fork  giving  d  145*45  at  52°  F.  French  pitch 
d=  145*2  vib. 

There  was  also  a  monoohord  on  which 
many  scales  were  indicated,  and  two  sets  of 
reed  pitch-pipes,  which  cannot  be  described 
for  want  of  space. 

Modem  Greek  Scale,  According  to  Me- 
shaqah,  in  Eli  Smith  (op.  cit  p.  264,  note  §), 
the  modern  Greeks  divided  the  Octave  into 
4  X  17  «  68  parts,  and  form  the  scale  by  12,  9, 
7,  12,  9,  7,  12  of  these  divisions.  Since,  then, 
1 200 -^  68=  17*65  cents,  the  scale  in  cents  will 
be  o,  212,  371,  494,  706,  865,  988,  1200,  which 
again  has  a  neutral  Third  and  Sixth,  371*865. 
If  the  scale  had  consisted  of  12,  8,  8,  12,  8.  S, 
12  of  these  divisions,  we  should  have  got  the 
precise  scale  of  Villoieau  (p.  520a'  1.  5),  which 
is  a  singular  additional  justification  of  iiis  divi- 
sion of  the  Octave  into  1 7  equal  parts. 


Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


INDEX. 


The  nnmben  refer  to  the  pages ;  the  letters  a,  b,  c,  d  to  tlie  quarters  of  a  page,  and,  when  there  are  donble  colomns, 
of  the  first  column.  In  which  care  a',  V^  d,  d\  refer  to  the  qoarterw  of  the  rarond  column. 
*  before  the  number  of  the  pa^  shewn  that  the  title  uf  a  book  or  paper  is  there  olted. 
t  ]  indicate  notes  and  aiditions  by  the  Translator. 


A«  X,  A",  see  VoweU 

[Aalst,  Van,  on  Chinese  Music,  524c] 

Abdul  Eadir,  same  as  Abdulqadir,  Persian,  f  4th 
cent.,  282a.  his  17  tones,  282a,  6,  and  12 
scales,  282  to  283 

Abdulqadir,  2816',  cf,  282^.  [his  16  Fifths, 
281c'.  his  system,  282^,  3646,  523a] 

Accented  note  names,  how  related  to  pitch 
numbers,  i6c 

Accidental  Scales  defined,  267a 

r Acoustical  facts  in  change  of  key,  55 li] 

Acoustics,  physical  and  physiological,  16,  its 
connection  with  music,  ic.  has  hitherto  not 
helped  musical  theory,  i^.  physical,  a  section 
of  the  theory  of  elasticity,  3c.  physiological, 
investigates  processes  in  the  ear,  4a.  its 
physical  part,  46 

Added  Sixth,  chord  of,  294^,  or  imperfect 
minor  triad,  344c 

Addition,  algebraical,  of  waves,  of  velocities 
and  displacements,  2^d^  note 

[Africa,  Western,  Balafongs,  5186] 

[Air-reed,  aerial,  or  aero-plastic  reed,  397c] 

[Alboni,  her  just  intonation,  4786] 

Alexander  the  Great,  zyid' 

Al  Farabi,  282a  [his  Greek  scales  after  Pro- 
fessor Land,  515c] 

[Alternative  Intervals  varying  by  Quartertones, 
in  modern  Arabic  and  Indian  scales,  524c] 

[Amati  of  Cremona,  1 596- 1684,  resonance  of 
his  violins,  876] 

Ambrose  of  Milan,  a.d.  374-398i  his  authentic 
scales,  243a  [doubtful  whether  they  were 
really  his,  z^id'.]  his  numerical  notation  of 
the  modes,  2696.  his  ecclesiastical  scales,  to 
be  regarded  as  essential,  2716,  c 

Ambrosch,  Chinese  melodies,  *2$%d\  26id' 

r Amels  possesses  Scheibler's  Tonometer,  444a] 

[America,  U.S.  of,  511a] 

[American  Organ  Reeds,  554^'] 

[Amiot,  ♦95^',  262d,  548c] 

Amplitude,  loc,  346 

Ampullce,  see  Ear 

Analysis  of  compound  and  composite  tones 
into  simple  vibrations  by  the  ear,  33.  this  is 
independent  of  power  to  analyse  vibrational 
forms  by  eye,  34fi{.  of  air  into  pendular  vibra- 
tions by  sympathetic  resonance,  independently 
of  ear,  42^2.  objective,  48a,  6.  of  sensations, 
its  difficulty,  496.  of  compound  into  simple 
tones  by  ear,  its  theory,  148c 

Anche  lihre^  or  free  reed,  956 

Ansa,  presumed  Indian  tonic,  see  Vadi,  2436 

Antony,  ♦239^ 

Anvil,  see  Ear 


Appogiatura,  always  makes  a  Semitone,  2876 

Approximation  in  pitch,  forms  a  musical  con- 
nection, 352& 

Appunn,  late  of  Hanau,  his  high  pitch  from 
forks,  x8a,  used  by  Preyer,  1 5  ic.  his  pipes  for 
artificial  vowels,  127^,  128a.  suggestion  to 
Preyer,  iSyd,  his  loaded  reeds,  176^,  177^'. 
[their  pitch,  as  determined  by  Translator, 
1776.  his  conical  resonators,  373d.  [his  reed 
tonometer,  443a.  his  difficulty  in  tuning  a 
series  of  perfect  Fifths,  483^2] 

Aqtueductus  vestibuli,  its  function,  136a 

Arabesques  compared  to  music,  252c 

[Arabia  and  Persia,  scales  after  Professor  Land, 
515^.  modem,  after  Eli  Smith,  515^.  medieval 
scales  after  Professor  Land,  with  7  and  8 
tones,  516a,  6,  5i9<f.  lute,  earlier  notes  after 
Professor  Land,  5166,  520^.  medieval  notes 
after  ditto,  517a,  520a] 

Arabic  Scales,  282  to  283.  modem,  of  24 
Quarter-tones,  2646.  [according  to  Professor 
Land,  284  note] 

Arabic  and  Persian  musical  system,  28o&to  2856. 

Arabs  have  no  pleasure  in  polyphonic  music, 
1966 

Architecture,  its  analogy  to  musical  composi- 
tion, 2c.  the  periods  of  its  progress  are  the 
analogues  of  those  of  music,  235c 

Archytas  first  settles  major  l%ird  as  4:5, 
262c,  3626,  d 

Aristotle,  on  Consonance,  237a,  />,  on  variations, 
2376.  his  indications  of  a  tonic,  240c,  24i{2,^', 
indications  of  a  downward  leading  note,  242a, 
251C,  d,  the  only  writer  who  indicates  a 
tonic,  2676.  his  conception  of  effects  of 
music,  251a.  makes  mese  tonic,  268a,  d,  on 
descending  leading  note,  286c,  [d] 

Aristoxenus,  his  twelve  Fifths,  271a  [5486] 

[Armes,  Dr.,  502a] 

Art,  works  of,  must  not  display  their  purpose, 
366c 

Artificial  compound  tones,  120c  to  122^2 

Artificial  production  of  vowels,  or  vowel  synthe- 
sis, by,  123a  to  124.  [tabular  statement  of 
results,  1 24^:2.]  by  organ  pipes  when  the  effect 
of  difference  of  phase  is  not  under  investiga- 
tion, 12yd,  Appunn*B  pipes  for  this  purpose, 
128a 

Artusi  blames  Monteverde  for  using  dominant 
Seventh  without  preparation,  248^ 

Auditory  apparatus,  its  advantage,  1346.  the 
mechanical  problem  it  has  to  solve,  134^, 
how  solved,  135a,  &,  c.  sand,  137a,  stones  or 
otoliths,  of  fish,  139a,  of  crastaceans,  149c. 
oiliffi  of  ampu)lsB,  in  former  editions  sup- 
posed to  be  hearers  of  noises,  151a,  6,  may  be 
hearers  of  squeaking,  hissing,  <S;c.,  isid,  hairs 


Digitized  by 


^.joogle 


558 


INDEX. 


of  Mysis,  150a.  nerves,  how  excited,  hypothe- 
sis, 5a.    ossicles  described,  131a,  b 

[Austro-Hungary,  pitch,  504c] 

Authentic  scales  of  Ambrose  of  Milan,  242^2, 
243a.    Glarean's  six,  245c,  d,  the  first,  267a 


B 


[B  natural  and  B  flat,  ancient  signs  for,  Si2d] 

Bach,  C.  P.  Em.,  his  equal  temperament,  321c, 
considers  equal  temperament  the  most  per- 
fect intonation,  323c  [548^1 

Bach,  J.  Sebastian,  down  to  his  time  final 
chords  always  major,  or  without  the  Thirds, 
217a.  his  suites,  245a.  his  use  of  closing 
minor  chord,  296^2.  his  use  of  the  major 
Sixth  in  the  ecclesiastical  Doric  or  mode  of 
the  minor  Seventh,  3046,  305a.  [example 
analysed  by  duodenals,  304c,  d,  c'.]  his  use  of 
the  mode  of  the  minor  Sixth,  307c  [503c,  S^Sd"] 

[Bagdad  Tambour,  its  scale,  after  Prof.  Land, 
517c] 

[Bailey,  549a] 

Bajazet,  282a 

Ball,  struck  up  as  it  falls,  its  periodic  motion, 
i^,  21C,  and  fig.  9 

Barrow,  *95d',  262d 

Basevi,  352c,  ♦352^ 

Basilar  membrane,  1 38a.  Hensen  and  Basse's 
researches  on,  145&.  its  breadth  probably 
determines  the  tuning,  14  Sh.  high  notes  near 
round  window,  low  notes  near  vertex,  146c. 
breads  easily  along  radial  fibres,  not  trans- 
versely, 146a.  consequent  mathematical 
theory,  1466.  its  fibres  form  approximatively  a 
series  of  stretched  strings,  146c.  its  behaviour 
for  noises,  mathematicfJly  investigated,  403c, 
its  vibration  in  the  cochlea  mathematically 
investigated,  4o6d 

Bass,  figured,  shows  new  view  of  harmony,  248c 

Bass  notes  with  tinkling  upper  partials,  11 65 

Bassoon,  its  tongue  or  reed,  966.  conical  tube, 
produces  all  harmonics,  99a  [reeds,  554c] 

Bausoh,  his  violin,  85c 

Beats,  5a,  of  simple  tones,  how  distinguished 
from  combinational  tones,  159^2.  their  origin, 
their  frequency  :=  the  difference  of  pitch  num- 
bers of  generators,  164^.  diagram  of,  165a. 
examples,  1656,  c.  from  upper  partials  as 
well  as  primes,  165c.  rendered  visible, 
1 65(2.  require  the  sympathetic  body  to  be 
nearly  of  the  same  pitch  as  itself,  i6sd, 
what  becomes  of  them  when  too  fast  to  be 
counted,  i66d.  according  to  T.  Young,  they 
become  the  differential  tone,  i66d  to  167a. 
objections  to  this  hypothesis,  1676.  [cheap 
apparatus  for  shewing  167 d\  use  of  Har- 
monical  for  shewing,  i6Sd,]  how  best  ob- 
served, 167c.  their  character,  z68a,  jarring 
like  letter  B,  168&.  intermittent  tones  heard 
with  a  reed  pipe  or  tuning-fork  and  double 
siren,  i68c.  produce  intermittent  excitement 
of  auditory  nerves,  169&.  do  not  disappear 
from  rapidity  only,  but  also  depend  on  in- 
terval, 170(2.  even  132  beats  in  a  second  are 
audible,  171a.  the  character  of  the  roughness 
alters  with  the  number  of  beats  in  a  second, 
17  ic.  beats  of  a  Semitone  heard  up  to  4,000 
vib.  per  second,  171c,  of  a  whole  tone  to  2,000 
vib.  per  second,  ijid.  major  and  minor 
Thirds,  smooth  from  264  to  528  vib.,  are 
rough  in  bass,  17  id,  their  roughness  does  not 
depend  solely  on  their  frequency,  I7i<2, 172a, 
but,  in  a  compound  manner,  on  magnitude  of 


interval  and  frequency,  172a.  on  the  siren 
will  determine  whether  the  note  heard  is  the 
prime  or  an  upper  partial,  i74d.  from  the 
upper  partials  of  a  single  tone,  1786  [I78d']. 
of  upper  partials  of  two  compound  tones,  i8oa, 
examples,  i8oc.  why  consonances  produce 
no  beats,  and  why  if  they  are  slightly  altered 
beats  ensue,  18 16.  of  disturbed  consonances, 
how  observed  with  double  siren,  182c.  of 
upper  partials,  their  rapidity  has  a  pre- 
ponderating influence  on  distinctness  of 
definition,  184&.  law  for  determining  them, 
with  tables,  184c,  d,  the  amount  of  dis- 
turbance of  a  consonance  being  the  same, 
the  beats  increase  with  the  higher  numbers 
expressing  it,  185a.  table  of,  when  consonances 
are  altered  by  a  Semitone,  185c.  due  to  com- 
binational tones,  197c.  of  combinational 
tones,  can  alone  distinguish  consonance  from 
dissonance  of  simple  tones,  199&.  of  differen- 
tial tones  cannot  occur  if  consonant  interval 
ratios  are  exactly  observed,  but  occur  instantly 
if  they  are  not,  203c.  peculiar  character  of 
those  with  bowed  instruments,  208&.  of  the 
tempered  triad,  3226,  c.  their  effect  on  its 
harmoniousness,  322^.  variation  in  the  pitch 
of  the  beating  tones,  414c.  calculation  of 
their  intensity  according  to  the  intervals  of 
the  beating  tones,  415^2.  [how  to  count, 
444^] 

[Beats  and  Ck>mbinational  Tones,  recent  "works 
on,  sect.  L.  see  table  of  contents,  p.  527] 

Beauty,  subject  to  laws  dependent  on  human 
intelligence,  366 

B6dos,  Dom,  *i6c  [his  4  old  French  foot  pipe, 
1 6c  note,  494^,  50&1.  knows  only  mean  tone 
temperament,  548c] 

Beethoven  [uses  pianofortes  by  Slein,  77(2], 
his  use  of  the  mode  of  the  minor  Sixth,  308ft. 
his  relation  to  equal  temperament,  32  7(; 

[Behnke,  Emil,  *iood\  *ioid',  on  registers  of 
voice,  1 01 6] 

[Belgium,  pitch,  504^] 

[Bell,  Graham,  inventor  of  Telephone,  finds 
and  demonstrates  double  resonance  in  all 
vowels,  107(2.  his  paper  on  Vowel  Theories^ 
*io8(2] 

[BeU,  Melville,  *io$d,  his  vowel  system,  io5(i, 
107(2] 

Bellermann,  *26sd' 

BeUs,  large,  how  set  swinging  by  periodical 
efforts,  36d.  their  inharmonic  proper  tones, 
72c.    why  they  beat,  73a 

Bell-shaped  glasses,  broken  by  singers,  39(2 

[Belly-bridge  of  piano,  old  single,  77^.  the 
divided,  was  introduced  by  John  Broadwood, 
1788,  77cr\ 

[Bender,  505a] 

Bemouilli,  Daniel  (1700-1782),  on  law  of  mo- 
tion of  strings,  15a  [441c] 

[Best,  W.  T.,  organist,  500c] 

[Bettini,  507c] 

[Bevington,  organ-builder,  5o6e] 

[Bishop,  organ-builder,  506c] 

[Bitter,  Life  of  J.  S.  Bach,  548(2] 

[Black  Digital  Scales,  518&,  5226^ 

Blade  of  air,  blade-shaped  lamina  of  air  aft 
mouth  of  flue-pipe,  its  action,  92a,  395a 

[Blahetka,  on  Vienna  pitch,  504^] 

[Blaikley,  D.  J.,  on  velocity  of  sound  in  tabes, 
*90(2.  distance  of  plane  of  reflexion  from  end 
of  flue-pipe,  *gid,  action  of  lips  in  blowing 
the  horn,  &o.,  97(2.  office  of  the  air  in  the 
tube  in  relation  to  the  lips,  *97d%  gSd^  990, 
*  c'  d\    his  account  of  the  clarinet  and  its 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


INDEX. 


559 


partials,  996,  e.  how  brass  tabes  for  horns 
are  shaped  and  bent,  99^.  their  shape  not 
truly  conical,  99^.  sounds  harmonics  on 
^French  horn  in  exact  tune,  99^'.  form  of 
trumpets,  looc.  two  lowest  partials  out  of 
tone,  looc.  length  of  French  horn  with  its 
various  crooks,  lood.  trombone,  its  shape, 
1 00c'.  slide  trumpets,  looc'.  keyed  horns 
obsolete,  looc'.  piston  horns,  lood',  on  side 
holes,  105^2,  210(2.  on  horns  being  in  just 
intonation,  327^.  on  the  conical  tubes,  tried 
by  Professor  Helmholtz,  394^,  note,  says 
there  are  no  ideal  brass  instruments  in  prac- 
tice, 42Sd,    forks,  494d] 

[Bodin,  so&d] 

[Boehm,  Theobald,  on  flutes,  *i03d.  English, 
edited  by  W.  8.  Broadwood,  103d'] 

Boethius,  on  the  old  tuning  of  the  lyre,  355c, 
266c 

Boltzmann,  93a 

[Boston  Music  Hall,  96^ 

Bosanquet  [his  resonator,  43^',  374c.  on  the 
measurement  of  intensity  of  sound,  *7Sd. 
distance  of  plane  of  reflexion  from  end  of  flue- 
pipe,  *gid.  Becent  work  on  combinational 
tones  and  beats,  152^2,  155^2',  156(2,  157(2, 
1^7(2,  *322(2.  his  use  of  Mercator*s  cycle  of 
53*  328c,  (2.  his  arrangements,  329a,  [329^2, 
d'],  his  manual,  429.  his  cycle,  4366',  its 
synonymity  and  intonation,  439.  his  gene- 
ralised fingerboard  and  harmonium,  4796  to 
481a.  on  beats  and  combinational  tones,  528 
to  538.  on  J.  S.  Bach's  equal  temperament, 
548(2] 

[Bosch,  his  Seville  organ,  511a] 

Bourget,  J.,  *73(2' 

Bowed  instruments,  190.  their  musical  tones, 
80c,  2076.  their  use  in  harmony,  207c.  not 
suited  for  soft  melodies  or  sustained  chords, 
3o8a.  their  scraping  character,  208c.  harsh- 
ness in  quartetts  by  good  players  not  accus- 
tomed to  play  together,  208c.  [one  of  its 
causes,  2oSd.  develop  combinational  tones 
well,  208(21  see  Violin,  Ac,  2076 

[Branchu,  Madame,  508c] 

Brandt,  cited,  on  Young's  law,  536.  modifica- 
tion of  his  experiment,  53c,  *syd 

Brass  instruments  have  sluggish  attack,  67a.  not 
suited  to  harmonies  except  out  of  doors,  210c 

Breguet,  his  watch-key  resembles  the  articu- 
lation of  anvil  and  hammer  in  the  ear,  133a 

[Broadwood,  James,  in  z8ii,  advocates  equal 
temperament,  548c] 

[Broadwood,  J.,  A  Sons,  497a,  Soyd] 

[Broadwood,  John,  introduces  divided  belly- 
bridge  on  piano,  1788,  and  uniform  striking- 
place,  77c] 

[Broadwood,  Henry  Fowler,  present  head  of 
firm  of  J.  Broadwood  &  Sons,  has  introduced 
the  ^-length  striking  distance  on  all  his 
pianos,  77(21 

[Broadwood,  W.  S.,  his  edition  of  Boehm,  103(21 

[Brown,  Colin,  on  characteristics  of  Scotch 

music,  259(2  note,    on  WiU  you  go,  lassie, 

262(2'.    his  Voice  Harmonium  described  and 

figured,  326(2',  47od] 

[Browne  and  Behnke,  on  Voice,  Song%  and 
Speech,  *iood'] 

Brumel,  Anton,  2g6d 

[Bryceson,  5056,  5066,  c] 

[Burmah,  Patala  and  Balafong,  518a] 

[Byfield  A  Green,  506a] 

[Byfield<j^  Harris,  506a] 

[Byfield,  Jordan,  A  Bridge,  5066] 

[Byolin^  498a] 


Caccini  in  1600  invents  recitative,  248c 

Cadence,  complete  and  imperfect  or  plagal, 
2935,  examples,  293c 

Cairo,  Quartertones  used  there,  2656 

[Callcott  on  extreme  sharp  Sixth,  *^oSd] 

Canals,  see  Semicircular 

Canonic  imitation  developed  early  in  12th  cen- 
tury, 244c 

Cantus  firmus,  shews  leading  note,  287c 

Carissimi,  307^^ 

Catgut  strings,  their  quality  of  tone,  80& 

[Cavaill4-Goll,  Aristide,  organ-builder,  his 
rules  for  finding  pitch  of  flue  organ  pipes, 
*^,  (2.  tried  and  abandoned  free  reeds, 
96c.  builds  Mr.  Hopwood's  organ,  96(2.  on 
Boehm's  flutes,  I03(2'.  first  draws  attention 
to  the  mode  in  which  flue-pipes  speak,  397a'. 
his  *  soufilerie  de  pr^ision,'  4426, 494(2',  508(1 

[Cavallo,  Signor,  helps  J.  Broadwood  with  the 
divided  belly-bridge,  77c'] 

[Cell,  harmonic,  or  Unit  of  Concord,  458c] 

[Centesimal  Cycle,  437&1 

[Cents  or  hundredths  of  an  equal  Semitone, 
41(2.  how  to  calculate,  App.  XX.  sect.  C,  see 
contents,  446c] 

[Challenger,  5496] 

Changing  or  passing  notes,  353a 

[Chappell,  507(2] 

[Character  of  each  tone  in  the  major  scale, 
according  to  Curwen,  279c,  d] 

Characters  of  keys,  are  there  any  absolute  ? 
3 IOC.  differ  on  pianos  and  violins,  why, 
3 1 1  c,  and  on  wind  instruments,  311c.  may  be 
influenced  by  peculiar  resonance  of  ear  pas- 
sage, 3 1 1  c.    [presumed,  after  E.  Pauer,  5506] 

[Chev6,  Emile,  425(2,  marries  Nanine  Paris, 
writes  theory  of  Galin-Paris-Chev6  method, 
his  29  division  of  the  Octave,  4250',  436(2.  his 
system  contrasted  with  Tonic  Sol-fa,  426(2] 

[Chev6,  Mme.,  formerly  Nanine  Paris,  425c', 
her  principles  of  teaching  to  sing,  4266I 

[Chickering,  5oi(2] 

[China,  scales  from  instruments  and  musicians 
at  the  Health  Exhibition,  51 8(2,  5226] 

Chinese,  their  numerical  speculations  on  music, 
229c.  their  pentatonic  scales,  257(7.  learned 
heptatonic  scales  under  Tsay-yu,  258a.  their 
pentatonic  airs  are  dull,  258(7.  260a.  261 6. 
[their  equal  temperament,  548c.  their  free 
reeds,  554^2] 

Chladni,  1756- 1827,  his  sand  figures  on  elastic 
plates,  41  &,  71(2,  the  proper  tones  of  such 
plates,  J2a 

Chord  defined,  24a,  211c.  of  four  parts  a 
tetrad,  2226.  rules  for  open  and  close  posi- 
tions hitherto  given  had  no  theory,  224c 

Chords,  growth  of  feeling  for  their  relation- 
ship, 292(2,  293a,  2966.  of  the  tonal  modes 
with  double  intercalary  tones,  297c,  (2,  with 
single  Do,  2986,  c.  of  Sixth  and  Fourth  J, 
and  of  Sixth  and  Third  J  or  Sixth  only  6, 
213a.  of  extreme  sharp  Sixth,  their  Gtreek 
Doric  cadence,  2866.  of  the  Seventh  used 
to  connect  other  chords,  3576.  see  also 
Closing  Chords,  and  see  Italian,  German,  and 
French  Sixth,  diminished  Seventh,  iftc. 

Chordal  sequences,  355c 

Chordal  relationship  felt  in  15th  and  i6th 
centuries,  369a 

Chrysanthus  of  Dyrraohium,  Archbishop,  de- 
clares Greeks  have  no  pleasure  in  polyphonic 
muFio,  and  leaves  it  to  the  West,  196&, 
♦196(2 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


56o 


INDEX. 


CilisB  of  ampullffi,  see  Auditory  CiliaB 
Cithara,  five-stringed  lute,  2Syd 
['  Clang/  used  by  Prof.  Tyndall  for  compoond 
tone,  why  not  so  used  here,  24d.    Webster's 
definition  of,  24d] 

['  Clangtint,'  used  by  Prof.  Tyndall  for  quality 
of  tone,  why  not  so  used  here,  246'] 
[Clark,  Lieut.,  see  Macleod] 
Clarinet,  its  tongue  or  reed,  966.    cylindrical, 
with  unevenly  numbered  partials,  98c.     [D. 
J.  Blaikley's  account  of  the  clarinet  as  not 
wholly    cylindrical,    996,    c]      its    peculiar 
action  in  forming  chords,  2iod.    experiment 
with  it  and    just    harmonium,   211a.     [its 
tones,  392c.    its  reeds,  553c] 
[Clavichord,  strikes  the  string  at  end,  ySd'] 
Closing    Chords,    their   development,    290^. 
their  five  major  forms,  291c.    major  chords 
in  minor  modes,  296^2,  297a 
Cochlea,  see  Ear 

Cochlean  nerve,  its  expansion,  139&,  e 
[Cohen,  5086] 
[Colbacchini,  510c] 
CoUard,  507^,  5496 

[Colour,  used  by  T.  Young  for  quality  of  tone, 
why  not  so  used  here,  240^ 
Coloured  lights,  mixture  of,  different    from 
mixture  of  pigments,  646 
Colourings,  Greek,  XP^^  t^eir  reality,  2656 
Colours,    primary,    scarlet-red,    yellow-green, 
blue-violet,  646,   [d  note]  never  seen  pure, 
64c.    power  to  distinguish  generally  absent, 
640^.    analysis  of,  into  three,  by  Waller,  94a 
Combinational  tones,  5a.    occur  when  the  vi- 
brations of  the  air  are  not  infinitesimal,  152c. 
result  from  all  the  partial  tones,  153&.    most 
easily  heard  when  generators  are  less  than 
an  Octave  apart;    for  harmoniums  can  be 
reinforced  by  a  resonator,  but  in  other  cases 
not,  153c.    [heard  simultaneously  with  rattle 
of  beats,  153c.  from  two  flageolet  fifes,  153d.] 
multiple,  considered  as  of  different  orders, 
iS4d,    [not  all  audible,  155^'.]    once  thought 
to  be  subjective,  and  to  result  from  beats  be- 
coming too  rapid  to  be  heard  separately,  156c. 
objections  to  this  theory,  156c.    they  arise 
from  the  largeness  of  the  vibrations,  156(2. 
condition  for  being  well  heard  on  harmonium 
and  polyphonic  siren,  157^1  b,    may  be  gene- 
rated in  the  ear  by  unsymmetrical  form  of 
drumskin,  1586,  and  loose  joint  of  hammer 
and  anvil,  158c.    produce  tingling  in  the  ear, 
and  are  strong  when   soprano  voices  sing 
Thirds,  1580.    an  accessory  phenomenon  by 
which  beats  are  not  interrupted,  1676.     beat, 
197c.    delimit  consonances  when  the  partials 
do  not  suffice,  201 5.    the  most  general  cause 
of  beats,  2046.    important  for  the  harmoni- 
ousness  of  a  chord,  214c.  those  of  major  and 
minor  triads,  215a,  &,  essentially  different, 
214c.  their  mathematical  theory,  411(2.  their 
effect  in  Just,  Pythagorean,  and  Tempered 
chords    compared,   31 46  [and   note],    their 
beats  mathematically  investigated,  4186  to 
419c.    their  origin,  41 9(2,  in  the  siren  and 
harmonium,  419(2,  4^cx2 
[Commas  higher  indicated  by  superior,  lower 
by  inferior  figures,  proposed  by  Translator 
and  here  used,  2776,  c] 
Compass  of  instruments,  [lydlt  i8a,  [186,  c] 
[Compass  of  the  Human  Voice,  544^1 
[Composite  and  compound  tones  distinguished, 
33^1 
Composition  of   simple  tones,  tone  and  its   , 
Octave,   306,   c,  tone  and  its  Twelfth,  32.  ] 


artificial,  of  simple  tones  how  arranged,  l2oe 

to  122(2 

Compound  tones,  22a,  and  see  Tone 
Concatenation,  or  musical  connection  of  tones, 
direct,  3502,  indirect,  3516 
[Concertina,  Just  English,  470&] 
Concords  are  consonant  chords,  2ii<2 
[Condissonant    triads    defineci,    2ii(2',    35812 
note  t,  459«] 

Congregational  singing,  its  results,  2460 
Connection,  musical,  or  ooncatenation  of  notes 
in  the  scale,  350(2  to  352 
Consecutive  Twelfths  and  Fifths,  why  forbid- 
den, 359(2  to  361C.    Octaves,  why  forbidden, 
359c 
Consonances  result  from  coinoidenoes  of  upper 
partials,   iSzd,    tables  of    such   ooincident 
partiab,    183a.    also  in  musical    notation, 
183(2.    disturbed  by  the  consonances    next 
adjoining  them  in  the  scale,  meaning  of  this 
expression,  i86(2.    defined,  1946.    and  disso- 
nances, their  boundary,  2286,  which  has  not 
been  constant,  228(7.  absolute,  Octave,  Twelfth, 
and  double  Octave,  1946.    perfect.  Fifth  and 
Fourth,  194c.  medial,  major  Sixth  and  major 
Third,  194c.  imperfect  minor  Third  and  minor 
Sixth,  194(2.   great  diversity  of  opinion  on  the 
order  of  their  relative  harmoniousness,  196&. 
order  of  de  Vitry  and  de  Muris,  Franco  of 
Cologne,  and  Glarean's  Dodecachordon,  196c 
here  it  is  based  on  their  independent  har- 
moniousness, 197a.    their  influencse  on  each 
other,  tabular  views,   187.    separately  con- 
sidered, iSyd  to  1906 
Consonant    intervals,  why    so    called,    181c. 
[their  beating  partials  and  the  ratios  of  those 
partials  compared,  191 6,  c] 
Consonant  triads   not    exceeding  an  Octave 
examined,  only  six  possible  forms,  2 1 2d  to 
217a.    exceeding  an  Octave,  examined,  2176 
to  2226.    effect  of  transposition,  2180-2196 
Consonants,  tenues  and  fnedue,  their  charac- 
ter, 66a,   h.     hisses,  F  V,  <tc.,  R  and  L, 
67(2 
Comu  and  Mercadier,  experiments  on  yiolin 
intonation,  *32$d 
[Correlative  Duodene,  4626] 
Corti,  the  Marchese,  his  formations,  or  arches, 
or  rods,  1396,  d  to  I4id,    seem  most  suited 
for  sympathetic  vibration,   1456.    they   in- 
crease in  size  as  they  approach  the  vertex  of 
the  cochlea,  145(2.    their  alteration  of  fonu, 
146(2.    probably  play  only  a  secondary  part 
in  the  function  of  the  cochlea,  146(2.     viewed 
as  the  means  of  transmitting  the  vibrations 
of  the  basilar  membrane  to  the  terminals  of 
the  nerves,  147a.    may  be  4500,  or,  throwing 
off  300  for  extramusical  tones,  4200  in  the 
octave,  1476.    how  they  may  determine  pitch 
continuously,    147a.    mode   in   which    they 
analyse  tones,  147c  to  148&.    may  vibrate  to 
two  tones,  and  their  vibrations  may  be  com- 
pounded of  them,  1 66c.    attempt  to  estimate 
intensity  of  sympathetic  vibration    for   in- 
creasing intervals  of  tones,  172c.    choice  of 
hypotheses,  172(2.    considered  as  explaining 
consonance  and  dissonance,  2276 
[Costa,  Sir  M.,  502a,  507c,  SSSdj 
[Couchet,  Jan,  Antwerp,  harpsichord  maker, 
knew  that  striking-pliu>e  affects  quality,  77c'. 
Coussemaker,  *  196(2,  *243(2',  *244d 
[Cramer,  5056] 
[Crawford,  526a] 

[Cross  and  Miller  on  American  pitch,  494c] 
[Crotch,  Dr.,  5496] 


Digitized  by  VjOOQIC 


IKDEX. 


S6i 


Crmnples  in  vibrational  form  of  vioUn  strings, 
f^b 

Cmstaoea,  observations  on  their  auditory  ap- 
paratns  by  V.  Hensen,  1490 

[Corwen,  John,  names  of  registers,  loie.  his 
use  of  the  character  of  a  tone  in  singing, 
*279c,  d.  on  major  Sixth  of  the  minor  scale, 
^joid.  his  work  with  Tonio  Sol-fa,  4246  to 
4256.    his  pitch,  496c] 

[Carwen,  John  Spencer,  eldest  son  of  John 
Corwen,  President  of  the  Tonic  Sol-fa  College, 
424^.  his  letter  to  the  Timest  424a'.  his 
ifemoriaU  of  John  Curwen,  425c] 

[Cycle  of  53,  how  it  arises,  465a] 

[C^rclic  Temperaments,  435^] 

Cjmdbalom  of  19  notes  to  the  Octave,  men- 
tioned by  Pnetorius,  320c.  value  of  the 
notes,  320(f 

[Caezmak,  on  whispering,  *io&i'] 


d'Alembert,  his  theory  of  consonance,  232a  to 
2336.  his  Pythagorean  Sixth  in  major  scale 
not  allowed,  2y$d.  says  Bameau*8  tuning 
was  common  in  1762,  32 1&.  his  explanation 
of  the  limits  of  the  Greek  heptachord,  35  id 

[d'Aligre,  Marquis,  5086] 

Damping,  rapid,  of  tones  of  air  in  mouth,  1 12c 
to  113a 

Damping  of  vibrations  in  the  ear,  142c  to  143d 

Danoe  music  in  form  of  madrigals  and  motetts 
in  Aj>.  1529,  down  to  J.  S.  Ba^  and  Handel, 
245a 

[Deoad,  harmonic,  or  Unit  of  Harmony,  4596] 

[de  Cans,  Solomon,  509c] 

Deep  tones  require  more  power  to  make  them 
audible  than  high  tones,  174&.  experiments 
dewing  how  weak  deep  tones  are,  1756.  below 
iiovib.  are  more  or  less  discontinuous  on  the 
siren,  178a.  jar  on  harmonium  below  132 
vib.,  178a.  produced  by  a  string  weighted  in 
the  middle,  176a.  37^  vib.  weak,  29^  vib. 
scarcely  audible,  1766.  Professor  Helmholtz, 
vrith  large  forks  of  24  to  35,  and  35  to  61 
vib.,  found  30  vib.  weak,  28  vib.  scarcely  audi- 
ble, 176c.  on  Appunn's  reeds,  Preyer's  ex- 
periments, I76d.  Author's  conjecture,  I76d\ 
{Translator's  experiments,  lyed'] 

XNsepest  musical  tone,  how  many  vibrations  it 
has,  174a 

Deepest  practical  orchestral  tone,  41}  vib., 
t7SC  , 

[Degenhardt,  5026] 

[de  la  Fage,  494£f,  509a,  510c] 

de  la  Tour,  Cagniard,  his  siren,  12c.  [494^', 
SoSc,d] 

Delaitre,  ssad 

de  la  Motte  Fouquet,  105a 

Delesenne  shows  that  first-class  violinists  play 
in  just  intonation,  325a,  ^3250,  title,  [his 
monochord,  44id',494d',  508a,  d,  d] 

de  Muris,  Jean,  his  consonances,  excludes 
Fourths,  1960 

[Demuth,  organist,  S4Sd\ 

[de  Prony,  494^',  5080] 

de  Vitry,  Philipp,  his  consonances,  excludes 
Fourths,  196c 

Diapason  normah  i6<2,  [512^'} 

[Diapason  work  of  organs,  93(2'] 

Diaphony  of  Huobald,  24^ 

[Dichord,  5236] 

[Diohordal  or  double  diatonic  scale  of  H.  W. 
Poole,  344«>  477c  to  478c] 

Didymna  included  major  Third  4 : 5  in  the 


syntono-diatonic  mode,  228c.    his  tetrachord, 
263a 
Differential  combinational  tones  (Surge's  and 
Tartini's) ,  1 53a.  of  usual  harmonic  intervals, 
154a.    generated  by  upper  partials,  154c.    of 
different  orders  exemplified,  155a,  6,  c,  [cal- 
culated, iSSd],    form  a  complete  series  of 
harmonic  partial  tones  up  to  the  generators, 
155c.    [influence  of  this  on  consonance  of 
simple  tones,  iSS^\  537^*]    their  reinforce- 
ment by  resonators,  often  small  and  dubious, 
I57d,  may  then  arise  from  vibrations  com- 
municated to  resonator  by  drumskin,  158a. 
of  the  first  order,  their  beats,  198a,  b»    beat 
only  when  upper  partials  beat  and  with  the 
same  frequency,  199a.  of  higher  orders,  their 
use  in  distinguishing  Fifths  and  Fourths  of 
simple  or  slightly  compound  tones,  201 .  [530a] 
[Digitals  a  finger-keys,  50^'] 
Dixninished  Seventh,  its  chord  and  transform- 
ations of  the  same,  345c,  d.    [its  chord  has 
the  just  form,   10  :  12  :  14  :  17,  considered, 
346c,    d.     its   transformations    considered, 
346c',  d',]    its  usual  just  chord,  3496 
[Dionysius  of  Halicamassus,  24fid] 
Direct  system,  its  chords,  342c 
Direction,  the  sensation  of,  is  partly  due  to 
muscular  sensations,  63d 
Discant  at  end  of  eleventh  century  in  France 
and  Flanders,  its  nature,  2446.  develops  poly- 
phonic music  and  musical  rhythm  and  ca- 
nonic imitation,  244c 

Dissonance  defined,  194&,  204&.  how  it  arises, 
330c,  whither  it  tends,  330(i.  different  for 
different  qualities  of  tone,  205a.  how  cha- 
racterised, 226c.  and  eonsonanoe,  their  boun- 
dary, 2286.  not  been  always  the  same,  228c. 
unprepared,  3540 

Dissonant  chords  imperfectly  represent  com- 
pound tones,  3466.  dissonant  notes,  ^6c.  es- 
sentially, why  used,  3536.  intervals,  why  so 
called,  18 ic.  considered,  general  view,  33 id 
to  333d.  notes  of  chords  of  the  Seventh,  con- 
sidered, 3476  to  3500.  tetrads,  341c.  tones  how 
introduced,  3536.  triad  C  E  A\>  or  C  E  0%, 
213d,  [2i4dr].  triads,  3386 
Disturbance  of  consonances  by  adjacent  con- 
sonances, meaning  of  this  expression,  i86d 
Division  of  small  intervals  into  equal  parts  by 
ear,  256a 

Dogs  very  sensitive  to  high  e""  of  violin,  i  i6d 
Dominant.    Seventh,  its  chord  not  used  in  1 6th 
century,  246^2.    [Duodene,  461^2] 
[Doncaster,  Schulze's  organ  at,  96(2] 
Donders,  first  draws  attention  to  noises  attend- 
ing vowels,  67(2,  io6cf,    first  discovered  vowel 
resonances,  *io86.    how  he  estimated  them, 
io8c.    his  vowel  resonances  compared  with 
Prof.  Hehnholtz's,  1096,  [♦i6&2T 
Donkin's  Acoustics,  377a 
Doric,  national  Greek  scale,  242a.    its  scale, 
267c,  305c.    Glarean's,  or  ecclesiastical,  245c. 
considered  as  the  mode  of  the  minor  Seventh, 

303c 

Double  Octave  an  absolute  consonance,  194& 
Double  Siren,  see  Siren  double 
Dove's  polyphonic  Siren,  13a,  14a 
[Driffield,  Bev.  G.  T.,  494(2,  5056] 
[DriUed  Beeds,  5556] 
Drum  and  Drumskin,  set  Ear 
Du  Bois  Beymond,  sen.,  his  vowel  trigram, 

1056,  ♦io5(2 
Ductus  ooMearis,  lyjc 
[Duiffoprugcar  (Swiss  Tyrol  ,Bologna,and  Lyons, 

1510-1538),  resonance  of  his  violins,  876,  0 

Digitized  by  ^oOJOgie 


S62 


INDEX. 


[Duodenal,  its  xneaiiing  and  use,  4656] 

[Duodenarium,  the,  463a.  how  constructed, 
463(2.    limits  of,  4646J 

[Duodenation,  462^2] 

[Duodene,  hannonio,  or  Unit  of  Modulation, 
461a] 

[Duodenes,  musical,  or  the  development  of  just 
intonation  for  harmony,  Sect.  £,  see  con- 
tents, 457<2] 


E 


Ear,  its  analysis  of  musical  tones  according  to 
the  law  of  simple  vibrations,  496,  52a.  espe- 
cially sensitive  from  2640  to  3163  vib.,  e""  to 
g''"y  I  i6a.  consequent  effect  when  bass  voices 
sing,  1 1 66.  how  it  apprehends  and  analyses 
compound  tones,  i2Sc.  construction  of,  de- 
scribed, 129c  to  142a.  its  labyrinth,  I2gdt 
135(2  to  137a.  fluid  of  the  same,  136&.  its 
membranous  labyrinth,  i^6d,  its  hammer, 
or  malletis,  figured  and  described,  13 1&.  its 
anvil,  or  incuSy  132^2,  and  its  anvil's  joint  with 
the  hammer,  133&.  its  stirrup,  133^2.  figured, 
134a.  its  stirrup's  attachment  to  the  oval 
window,  134a,  and  excursions,  134&.  its  sac- 
cuius t^Z^'  its  ampullcBti^Cd,  its  utriculuSf 
136(2.  its  sand,  137a.  its  cochlea,  137&  to 
142a.  sensitive  tor  f"'t  and  its  consequences, 
179a.  contrasted  with  eye  as  to  capabilities 
of  perceiving  waves,  29.  compared  with  eye 
in  its  apprehension  of  compound  vibrations, 
128c.  ear  can  analyse,  eye  cannot;  eye  can 
distinguish  all  forms,  ear  can  only  distinguish 
those  which  have  different  constituents,  I28d. 
compared  with  eye  in  analysis  of  compound 
sensations,  I48<2,  and  for  intermittent  irrita- 
tions, 173a,  6,  c.  compared  with  muscles  for 
intermittent  irritations,  173&.  [compared 
with  eye  and  muscles  in  timing  a  transit, 
I73<2,  (2'.]  its  windows,  see  Oval  and  Bound 
Ecclesiastical  scales  of  Glarean,  with  incorrect 
Greek  names,  245c,  d 
[Edison,  T.  A.,  his  phonograph,  539a] 
Egyptian  flute,  interpreted  by  F^tis,  2'jid 
Eight-stringed  Scales,  Lydian,  Phrygian,  Doric, 
Hypolydian,Hypophrygian  (Ionic),  Hypodorio 
(Eolio  or  liocrian),  Mixolydian,  2670.  [the 
same  with  the  intervals  in  cents,  268<;] 
Ekert,  •307(2' 

Eleventh  not  so  pleasant  as  Fourth,    1895 
[its  partials  compared  with    those  of    the 
Fourth,  i89(2],  196a 
[Elliot,  507a] 

Ellis,  A.  J.  [♦i6(2',  17(2,  56(2,  66(2',  ♦68(2,  ♦68(2', 
♦  io5(2',  1 140^,  ♦  147(2.    his  Alphabet  of  Nature^ 
1845,  first  makes  mention  of  Willis's  and 
Wheatstone's    experiments     and    theories, 
♦11 7(2,  ♦191&',  390a] 
Energetic  tone  of  voice,  how  produced,  115c 
Engel,  G.,  ♦ii2(2 
[England,  pitch,  505(2] 

Enharmonic  confusions  occur  in  just  intona- 
tion, 327^.    [organ  of  Gen.  T.  P.  Thompson, 

473c] 

Eolic  mode,  Glarean's,  245(2 
Eolic  (Hypodoric)  Greek  scale,  267c 
Equal  temperament,  circumstances  favourable 
to  it,  3226.  first  developed  on  the  pianoforte 
where  much  favoured,  323(1^.  its  defects  on  the 
organ,  323c,  and  harmonium,  324a,  on  violins, 
3246.  not  used  in  double  stop  passages,  324c. 
its  influence  on  musical  composition,  3276. 
Mozart  and  Beethoven,  327c.  [its  cycle,  4366. 


its  intonation,  437c.    its  synonymity,  4386. 

its  history,  548J 
[Erard,  507(2] 
Eratosthenes,  his  method  of  tuning  the  older 

chromatic  tetraehord,  262c 
Erse  have  learnt  heptatonic  Scales,  2586 
Essential  scales,  2676 
[Esser,  504^2] 
[Estdve,  436(2'] 
Esthetics,  Musical,  i& 
Esthetic  principles  modify  physical    in    the 

formation  of  scales,  234  to  236.    analysis  of 

works  of  art,  3666 
Esthonian  treatment  of  leading  note,  288a 
Euclid  on  consonance  and  dissonance,  226(2, 

[523a] 

Euler,  Leonard  (1707- 1783),  on  law  of  motion 
of  strings,  15a,  on  why  simple  ratios  please, 
1 5&.  his  theory  of  consonance  and  dissonance 
founded  on  integers  explained,  *22gd  to  2316. 
the  gap  he  left  filled  by  Prof.  Helmholtz,  231c 
[his  determination  of  pitch  numbers  by  a  string 
and  formula,  441c.    494/2'.    5iO(2] 

Eustachian  tube,  1306 

[Ewing,  J.  A.,  1 1 8(2'.  analysis  of  vowels  ^7 
Phonograph,  538^542,  see  Jenkin] 

Exner,  S.,  *i5i(2,  ^3726 

[ExperimentfU  Instruments  for  Just  Intonation. 
4666  to  483c,  see  contents,  4666.    pipes,  506c] 

Extreme  sharp  Sixth,  its  chord  and  Greek 
Doric  cadence,  2866, 308c 

Eye  contrasted  with  ear  as  to  oapabilitiea  of 
perceiving  waves,  29 


Fagotto,  see  Bassoon 

False  (Midence,  356c.  relations  forbidden, 
36i(2,  their  meaning,  362a,  often  found  in 
J.  S.  Bach's  chorales,  3626 

Farabi,  same  as  Al  Farabi,  (2.  950,  282a 

[Faraday,  5056] 

[Farey,  J.,  sen.,  controversy  with  James  Broad- 
wood,  548c'] 

Fessel,  I22(2,  377c,  his  resonance  tubes,  dimen- 
sions of,  377^ 

F6tis,*239(2,*240(2',*257(2.  adopts  term  &maZ»/y, 
240&.  on  pentatonic  scales,  257c  271(2.  his 
interpretation  of  an  Egyptian  flute,  271(2, 
2800'.    [his  story  of  Lemmens,  28o(2'j 

Fifth,  14S,  not  sensibly  disturbed  by  adjacent 
intervals,  i88c.  [its  partials  compued,  i88(2.] 
a  perfect  consonance,  194^.  of  simile  tones 
delimited  by  beats  of  differential  tones,  aoo(2. 
repetition  in  it  presents  new  elementa,  2546. 
used  in  modem  music,  254(2.  occurs  in  the 
scale,  255a.  [indicated  by  ±,  276(2'.]  grave 
or  imperfect  and  just,  3356.  false  or  di- 
minished, 335c.  superfluous  or  extreme  sharp, 
335(2.    see  also  Subminor  Fifth 

Fifths,  consecutive,  why  forbidden,  3590,  36o(2, 
3616 

fifths  and  Fourths  consonant  and  dissonant 
enumerated  and  considered,  3356 

Fifth  and  Fourth  triad,  3386 

Fifth  and  Sixth  triad,  3386 

Films  of  glycerine  soap  and  water  for  shewing 
vibrations  of  air  in  a  resonator,  374 

[Finaly  called  Umic  in  text,  267(2j 

Final  Chords,  see  Closing  Chords 

[Finlayson,  5<X)a] 

[Fischer,  494(2',  508c,  d] 

[Flatter  or  lower  tones  defined,  i  id'] 

Floroke,  on  vowel  resonance,  io8c',  iogc 
Digitized  by  V^OOQIC 


INDEX. 


5^3 


Flue-pipes  of  an  organ  open  and  stopped,  S&bt 

c.    [their  pitch  according  to  Cayaill6-Goll,  89c, 

€2.  effect  of  temperature  on  pitch  of,  89(/,  d'J] 
motion  of  air  inside,  for  open  pipes,  89c  to  906, 
for  stopped  pipes,  90c.  their  reduced  length, 
91 6.  distance  of  plane  of  reflexion  from  the 
end  of  pipe  according  to  Prof.  Helmholtz,  91&, 
[according  to  Bosanquet,  Loid  Bayleigh,  and 
Blaikley,  gid],  motion  of  air  at  mouth,  926. 

'narroii70r  stopped  cylindrical,  have  proper  tones 
corresponding   to    the    unevenly   numbered 
partials,  the  toider  not  so,  and  henc^  give 
prime  tone  almost  alone,  94a,  the  blowing  of 
them,  394a  to  3966.    see  Organ  pipes,  flue 
flute  pipes,  see  Flue  pipes 
Flutes,  bad  for  harmony,  joke  on  a  Flute 
concerto  and  concert,  good  in  combination 
with  other  instruments,  20$d,    [old  and  new, 
their  intonation,  ss$d'  to  556a] 
[Tolliano,  L.,  invento  the  meantone,  but  not 
the  meantone  temperament,  547<2] 

[Foot,  lengths  of,  in  different  countries,  their 

effect  on  pitch,  $120] 
force  of  tone,  loc.  its  measure,  lod,  of  sound, 

its  mechanical  measurement,  lod,  [75<i'] 
Forks,  tuning,  their  sympathetic  resonance, 
39^,  40a.  [generally  have  the  second  partial, 
54^.  conditions  of  not  having  any  partials, 
55^'.]  purified  from  secondary  tones  by  ajar, 
54£2,  or  a  string,  55c.    see  also  Tuning-fork 

Forkel,  ♦296^,  32 id,  ^32 id' 

[Forster  A  Andrews,  5006] 

Forte  and  PianOt  how  produced  on  organs,  94c 

Fortlage,  307c 

Four-part  chords,  2226 

Fourier  {1768- 1830),  his  law,  346.  its  acous- 
tical expression,  34c.  what  it  shews  and 
does  not  shew,  356.  mathematically  solves 
Pythagoras*s  problem,  229a 

Fourth,  14^.  chiefly  disturbed  by  major 
Third,  189&.  its  precedence  over  major  Third 
and  major  Sixth  principally  due  to  its  being 
the  inversion  of  the  Fifth,  189&.  a  perfect 
consonance,  194c.  why  formerly  not  con- 
sidered as  a  consonance,  196^.  between  two 
simple  tones  delimited  by  beats  of  diffe- 
rential tones,  20ca.  mode  of,  Greek  Ionic 
Ecclesiastical  Mixolydian  considered,  302c. 
[its  predominance,  524^] 

[France,  pitch,  508a] 

Franco  of  Cologne,  end  of  12th  century,  admits 
Thirds  as  imperfect  consonances,  190a.  his 
order  of  consonances,  196c 

[Francis,  508&] 

[Fraser,  his  organ,  first  commercially  issued  in 
equal  temperament,  since  burned,  5492/] 

Free  Beeds,  956.  [in  Chinese  Shdng,  6sd. 
not  used  in  English  organ  pipes,  96c. 
treated  hj  Mr.  Hermann  Smith,  554^] 

French  pitch,  i6d,  [Commission  on  pitch, 
494(2'.    Sixth,  461c] 

French  horn,  see  Horn 

Frequency  defined,  iia,  [iid] 

Fullah  negroes  have  pentatonic  scales,  257c 

Fundamental  major  and  minor  chords,  21 2d, 
bass,  294c 

[Furstenau,  M.,  4996] 


Gabrieli  Giovanni  of  Venice,  composer,  con- 
temporary with  Palestrina,  247c.    what  we 
miss  in  him,  2486,  296a 
Gaels  have  learned  heptatonio  scales,  2586 
[Pafori,  treatise  on  music,  1480,  *si2d] 


Galileo    (1564-1649)  on  laws  of   motion  of 

strings,  15a 

[Galin,  P.,  his  book  and  system,  425^.    adopts 

Huyghens's  cycle  of  31,  425^] 
Galin-Paris.Chev6  system  of  teaching  singing, 

42 5c.     [its  history  and  principles,  425^  note  f] 
Galleries  of  cochlea,  137 6 
[Gamelan  or  Javese  band,  how  it  plays,  526c] 
[Gand,  509a] 

[Gardiner,  Tonic  Sol-fa  teacher,  427c] 
[Gameri,  G.,  or  *  Joseph '  (Cremona,  1683- 

1745),  also  called  Guamerius,  resonance  of  his 

violins,  87c] 
[Gameri,  P.  (Mantua,  1701),  resonance  of  his 

violins,  87cQ 

Oeigen-principal  organ  stop,  93a,  c,  d 
Oemshom  organ  stop,  94a,  [94^] 
Gerbert,  ♦196^ 
[German  peculiarity  of  consonants,  66d,  d', 

habit  of  beginning  vowels  with  the  check  or 

Arabic  hamza,   I04£2'.    Sixth,  461c.    pitch, 

5096] 
[Gewandhaus  concerts  at  Leipzig,  pitch,  510&] 
Glarean  sometimes  allows  tenor  and  bass  to 

be  in  different  keys,  2  \$d,    his  Dodecachordon 

and  its  order  of  consonances,  *i96c.    his  six 

authentic  and  six  plagal  scales  with  false 

Greek  names,  245c,  d,    his  names  of  the 

modes,  269a 
Glass  harmonioon,  71a 
[Glazebrook's  electric  method  of  determining 

pitch,  4426] 
Gleitz,  organist,  on  Erfurt  bell  of  1477  and  its 

tones,  *j2d 
Glottis,  98a 
[Glover,  Miss   Sarah,  starts    the  system  of 

teaching  to  sing  developed  as  Tonic  Sol-^ 

by  John  Curwen,  424a] 
[Glyn  &  Parker,  maker  of  HandePs  Foundling 

Hospital  organ,  So$d] 

Goethe,  1749 -1832,  relied  on  mixtures  of  pig- 
ments, 64^ 
Goltz,  his  investigation  of  the  ciliie  of  am- 

pullffi,  leads  to  suppose  that  they  and  the 

semicircular  canals  serve  to  give  sensation  of 

revolution,  151 6 

[Got,  M.,  pronounces  oi«i  without  voice,  68d] 
Goudimel,   Claude,   a   Huguenot,  master    of 

Palestrina,  2476 

Graham,  G.  F.,  *258<2',  2&od'y  26id 
[Grave  harmonics  =  combinationid  tones,  153c 

note] 
[Gray,  Dr.,  helps  J.  Broadwood  with  divided 

belly. bridge,  77(f| 
[Gray  <fe  Davison,  506c,  507a.    in  1854  first 

send  out  an  organ  in  equal  temperament, 

549&Q 
[Great  Exhibition  of  1851    had  no  English 

organ  in  equal  temperament,  549a'] 
[Greatorex,  496c] 
[Greece,  old  tetrachords,  5i2<2,  519(2.     ditto 

afterAIFarabi,  512^,519(2.   scales,  5 14  to  5 15. 

most  ancient,  51 56.    later,  and  Al  Farabi's, 

5«5c.  Si9d] 
Greek  Music,  237.    tonal  system,  262  to  271. 

later  scale,  270c,  &.    [scales,  514  to  515a] 
Greeks   had    a  certain   esthetic   feeling   for 

tonality,  but  tmdeveloped,  242c 
[Green,  505(2] 
Gregory,  Pope,  a.d.  590-604,  his  settlement  of 

the  Liturgy,  little  more  than  established  the 

Boman  school  of  singing  of  Pope  Sylvester, 

239a.    inserts  accidental  scales  among  Am- 

brosian  essential,  27  id] 

[Griesbaoh,  J.  H.,  499^,  5056]  ,     ^.^^T^ 

Digitized  by  Vo:«ILiOy  IC 


5^4 


INDEX. 


[Grove,  Sir  O.,  ♦87(i'] 

[Orundton^  fundamental  tone  or  root  of  chord, 

Goadanini,  violin  by,  85c 
[Guamerios,  P.,  Sy&,  see  Gameri] 
Gu6roult,  165^,  note  and  414c.  [4^7^] 
Guido  d'  Arezzo,  b.  about  990,  351c. 
[Guillaume,  510^ 
Guitar,  746 


[Halberstadt  organ,  1361,  oldest  pitch  ascer- 
tained,  51 1&] 
[HalU,  Ch.,  502c] 

HaUstroem  considered  multiple  combinational 
tones  to  be  of  different  orders,  *  154^2,  413a 
Hammer,    moved  by  water-wheel,  its  periodic 
motion,  19c,  21c.    soft  and  elastic  for  pianos 
complicates  tiie  problem,  74c,  a  sharp-edged 
metalho,  rebounding  instantly,  excites  but  one 
point  and  produces  numerous  partials,  some 
more  intense  tiban  the  prime,  75a.    of  piano- 
forte, why  felted,  75c.    hard  and  soft,  their 
different  qualities,  78c.    see  also  Ear 
Hammer-Purgstall,  von,  on  Arabian   Music, 
3810,  <2 
Bdnt&hu  of  cochlea,  137& 
Handel  sometimes  concludes  a  minor  piece 
with  a  major  chord,  21 76.    his  suites,  245a. 
his  use  of  closing  minor  chord,  295a,  2g6d, 
his  use  of  the  mode  of  minor  Sixth,  307a,  6, 
[takes  a  chorus  from  Carissimi,  sojd  notef . 
his  fork,  496c,  5056] 
Hanslick,  £.,  on  tiie  Beautiful  in  Music,  26, 
2Sod 

[Harmon,  Mr.  J.  Paul  White's,  for  the  53  divi- 
sion of  ^e  Octave,  481 6] 
Harmonic  music,  modem,  how  characterised, 
246c.  inducements  for  the  change,  246c. 
distinguished  from  polyphonic  by  the  in- 
dependence of  chords,  296a.  relationship 
began  in  the  middle  ages,  368c.  Seventh,  see 
sub-minor  Seventh,  upper  partials  produced 
by  the  same  peculiarities  of  construction  of 
a  body  as  allow  combinational  tones  to  be 
heard,  15&I,  1590.  these  always  accompany 
a  powerful  simple  tone,  159c.  upper  par- 
tiiUs,  why  they  play  a  leading  part  in  the 
sensations  of  the  ear,  204a 
[Harmonical,  an  instrument  for  musical  ob- 
servation, 6c.  a  specially  tuned  harmonium, 
its  price  and  compass,  17&,  c,  d,  useful  to 
shew  the  existence  of  partial  tones,  22d, 
useful  for  shewing  increasing  frequency  of 
beats  with  increasing  pitch,  i$Sd.  full  de- 
scription, 466^2] 
Harmonicons  of  metal,  wood,  and  glass, 
7ia,&,c 

[Harmonics,  how  they  differ  from  harmonic 
upper  partial  tones,  24^'.  defined,  zsd, 
of  C66,  table  of  partials  of  the  first  sixteen, 
to  shew  how  they  affect  each  other  in  con- 
sonances, 197c,  d,  of  a  string  45  inches  long 
struck  at  )  length,  78^.  partials  of  a  piano- 
forte string  struck  at  one-eighth  its  length, 
545c.  of  a  violin  or  harp,  and  fading  har- 
monics of  piano,  24(2'.  Seventh  and  Seven- 
teenth introduced  into  harmony,  464c 
Harmony,  of  the  spheres,  solely  heard  by 
Pythagoras,  229c,  and  plays  a  great  part  in 
middle  ages,  22gd.  its  modern  principle, 
249a,  249c.  not  natural,  but  freely  chosen, 
2496,  gave  rise  to  a  richer  opening  out  of  | 


musical  art,  369a.  [absent  in  non-harmonic 
scales,  526c] 

Harmonium,  its  reeds,  956, 554a'.  with  24  notes 
in  just  intonation,  invented  by  Prof.  Helm- 
holtz,  its  system  of  chords,  316c.  its  meUiod 
of  tuning,  316^'.  [its  duodenary  arrangement, 
3170.]  its  system  of  minor  keys,  318a  and  d, 
its  contrast  with  tempered,  319c.  [just,  de- 
scribed, 470a] 

Harmoniousness  of  combinations  in  different 
qualities  of  tone,  194a 

Harp,  74&.    with  pedal,  3226 

[Harper,  trumpeter,  and  his  son,  had  a  slide 
trumpet,  loocT^ 

Harpsichord  [its  striking-place,  77^ 

[Harris,  B.,  arches  the  upper  lip  of  flue-pipes, 
397^',  505^ 

[Harris,  T.  and  B.,  505^] 

Harmonisation,  the  only  point  in  which 
modem  excels  ancient  music,  309a 

[Hart,  violin-maker,  assists  in  finding  reso- 
nance of  violins,  Sje] 

[Hartan,  monochord,  442a] 

[Haseneier,  maker  of  Dr.  Stone's  contrafagotto, 
553&Q 

Hasse,  C,  proves  that  birds  and  amphibia  have 
no  Gorti's  rodb,  I45(2',  i^Sd 

Hauptmann,  objects  to  a  theory  of  consonance 
and  dissonance  by  rationiJ  numbers  that  no 
sharp  line  can  be  drawn,  *22yd.  Prof.  Helm- 
holtz's  reply,  228a.  his  Pythagorean  Sixth 
in  minor  scale  not  allowed,  *2';$d,  his  nota- 
tion for  Fifths  and  Thirds,  276^2,  277c.  his 
reason  for  avoiding  closing  minor  chords, 
295c,  295(2',  3ioe2'.  his  opinion  on  the  Second 
of  the  Scale,  298^2.  his  minor  major  mode, 
305  6.  denies  that  there  is  any  difference  of 
character  to  keys  played  on  an  organ,  3iodL 
his  system  of  tones,  315c,  340a.  on  consecu- 
tive Fifths,  3602,  3616 

Hautbois,  see  Oboe 

[Haweis,  Bev.  H.  B.,  assists  in  finding  reao- 
nance  of  violins,  87c] 

[Haydn,  54801 

[Healey,  Mr.,  assists  in  finding  resonance  <d 
violms,  876',  </] 

Heidenhain's  Utanomdtort  its  action,  139a 

HiUcotrSma  of  cochlea,  137& 

Hehnholtz,  Prof.,  *6c\  i&2.  his  Optics,  iSd', 
♦91c,  ♦9&2',  io5<2,  106c',  109c,  nod,  d\  •iiid, 
117(2,  123(2,  *i34d,  ♦iS2(2',  153(2,  195(2',  23&2'. 
his  names  for  the  modes  explained,  269c. 
[taken  to  hear  Gen.  P.  Thompson's  organ  by 

the  Translator,  423c',  and  taken  by  ihe  same 
to  Mr.  Gardiner's  School  to  hear  Tonic  Sol-ta 
singing,  427c.    his  letter  to  J.  Curwen  about 

it,  427^ 

[Helmholtzian  Temperament,  435c] 

[Helmore,  Bev.  T.,  on  Gregorian  modes,  ^26^ 
Heilwag,  Gh.,  loScf,  [on  vowel  resonance,  109c] 
Hemony  of  Zfitphen,  17th  century,  his  require- 
ments for  bells,  72(2 

[Henfling's  cycle  of  50,  436(2'] 

Henle  found  where  greatest  increase  of  breadth 

of  Gorti's  rods  fell,  146a 
Henrici,  estimates  upper  partials  of  tuning- 
forks  too  low  by  an  Octave,  *62a 
Hensen,  V.,  researches  on  the  basilar  mem- 
brane, 145c,  *  1 45(2'.    auditory  apparatus  of 

crastaceiB,  ^1490 
Hensen,  4o6d 
Heptatonic  Scales,  2626 
[Herbert,  Geo.,  had  organ  tuned  equally,  5490'^ 
Herschel,  Sir  J.,  iiyd' 
Hervert,  J.,  *93(2'.    see  Mach 

Digitized  by  V^OOQIC 


INDEX. 


565 


Sexaohord  of  Guido  d*Arezzo,  3510 

lUichi-riki,  Japanese,  its  reeds,  553^1 

Sidden  Fifths  and  Octaves,  361c.    often  found 
in  J.  S.  Bach's  chorales,  3626 

High  voices  more  agreeable  than  low,  and 
why,  179c 

fHildebrand  of  Dresden,  495c] 

[Hill,  violin-maker,  assists  in  finding  resonance 
of  violins,  876,  c] 

[Hill,  Thomas,  organ-bnilder,  his  father  tried 
and  abandoned  free  reeds,  96c.    494^',  506c] 

[Hipkins,  A.  J.,  on  harmonics  of  a  Steinway 
piano  string,  76^2.  on  striking-place  of 
pianoforte,  hai^sichord,  and  spinet,  77c,  note, 
had  not  met  with  a  striking-place  at  |  leng&, 
yydf,  his  experiments  on  harmonics  of  a 
string  struck  at  ^  its  lemrth,  *ySd.  and  fur- 
ther experiments  on  its  partials,  *545c. 
assists  in  finding  resonance  of  violins,  87c', 
i83{f,  2ogd,d\  monochord,  442a,  507^.  assists 
in  determining  non-harmonic  scales,  51461  c, 
on  James  Broadwood's  equal  temperament, 
549a.  introduces  equal  temperament  at 
Broad  woods',  1846,  549c] 

[Hitchcock's,  early,  i8th  century,  spinet,  its 
striking-place,  jyd] 

[Holland,  pitch,  5106] 

Uomophonic  music,  237  to  243 

[Hopkins,  E.  J.,  on  the  organ,  cited,  *33<2,  Syef, 
d',  93c',  d,  94d,  d',  96d,  49&i,  5496] 

[Hopwood,  of  Kensington,  his  organ  built  by 
Cavaill6.Goll,  96^2] 

Horn,  function  of  the  air  in  its  tube  in  rela- 
tion to  lips,  97c  [after  Blaikley,  97<2'],  long 
nearly  conical  tube,  99a,  [not  quite  conical, 
99^*  gives  the  harmonics  only,  99a  [upper 
ones  true,  99^1.  action  of  hand  in  the  bell, 
looa.  [2  lower  harmonics  false,  looc.  its 
various  lengths  for  diHerent  crooks  after 
Blaikley,  and  error  in  reporting  Zamminer, 
iQod.]  keyed  horns,  icx)a,  [nearly  obsolete, 
lood'] 

[Hewlett,  496c] 

Hucbald,  Flemish  monk,  at  beginning  of  loth 
century,  244a 

Hudson's  Bay,  pentatonic  scales,  257c 

[Hnggins,  Dr.,  F.B.S.,  his  observations  of  effect 
of  increasing  tension  of  hairs  in  violin  bow, 
83^2.  on  position  of  touch  for  Octave  har- 
monic, *84(2.  on  function  of  sound  post  of 
violin,  *86c,  d,  assists  in  finding  resonance  of 
violins,  87c.  the  resonance  of  his  Stradivari 
of  1708.  %^d'] 

[HuUah,  239d,  499^.  505&] 

[Hutchings,  Plaisted,  A  Ck>.,  51 1&] 

Huyghens,  1629- 1695  [knows  that  striking- 
place  of  string  affects  quality  of  tone,  77a. 
his  harmonic  cycle,  436c] 

Hypate,  uppermost  string  in  position,  lowest 
in  pitch,  answered  to  dominant,  242a.  Greek 
music  ended  on  it,  2426 

Hypodoric  (Eolic  or  Loorian)  Scale,  2670 

Hypolydian  Scale,  2670 

Hypophrygian  (Tonic)  Scale,  267c 


InciUt  8ee  Ear 

[India,  Chromatic  and  Semltonic  Scales,  5170. 

partial    Scales  of   Bajah  B4m  P&l   Singh, 

Siyd] 

[Indian  Quartertones,  how  produced,  265^2'] 
Indirect  or  Beverted  System  of  Chords,  3420 
Instrumental  tones  accompanied  by  distinctive 

xkoises,  6S6. 


Instruments  with  inharmonic  proper  tones  not 
suitable  for  artistic  music,  73c.  effect  of 
bowing  and  damping  them,  73^ 

Intercalated  tones,  always  Semitones,  3520 

Interference  of  sound,  160 

Intermittent  excitement  of  nerves,  its  effect 
on  ear  and  eye,  169&  to  170c 

Intensity  of  sound,  how  measured,  y$d  [Bosan- 
quet's,  with  Preece  and  Stroh's  opinion  upon 
this  measurement,  75^,  d'] 

Interval,  [a  sensation,  measured  by  ratios  or 
cents,  13d,  their  names  distinguished  in  print 
by  capital  letters,  lyd'.]  of  Fifth  and  Fourth, 
14a,  major  and  minor  Third,  146,  major  and 
minor  Sixth,  14c.  all  hitherto  considered  so 
arranged  that  a  pair  of  their  partials  shall  beat 
33  times  in  a  second,  191a,  192a.  [all,  except 
Tnirds  and  Fifths,  indicated  by  ...  or  the 
number  of  cents,  276^'.  not  exceeding  an 
Octave,  expressed  in  Cents,  Sect.  D,  see  pre- 
fixed table  of  contents,  45id.  neutral,  525a, 
alternative,  5256] 

[Intonation,  Tempered  Pythagorean  and  Just 
compared,  313c.  unequally  just,  465a.  of 
vocalists  and  violinists,  486  to  7.  of  flutes,  555 
d'  to  556a] 

Inversions,  first  or  chord,  of  Sixth  and 
Fourth  S,  second  or  chord  of  Sixth  and  Third, 
.^,  213a.  on  what  their  harmonious  effect 
depends,  214a 

Inwards  striking  reeds,  976 

Ionic,  Glarean's,  245c.  Greek,  considered  as 
mode  of  the  Fourth,  302c.  (Hypophrygian) 
Scale,  267c 

[Ions,  496^] 

Irrational  intervals,  (Uoyo,  264a 

[Isawa,  director  of  the  Musical  Institute,  Tokio, 
Japan,  522^,  526c',  527a',  S5^^] 

Italian  melodies  rich  in  intercalated  tones, 
352c.    [Sixth,  461c] 

[Italy,  in  1884  officially  adopts  l/bAS^  "Pre- 
senting the  arithmetical  pitch  c"5i2,  497<2, 
49Sa,  prior  pitch,  510c] 


[Japan,  koto  tunings,  heptatonic  scales, 
Biwa,  519a,  6,  c,  522a',  V,  d.   scales,  ^S^ih^^ 

[Java,  music,  7 id,  237^2'.  its  pentatonic  scales, 
257^2.  Salendro  Scales,  518c,  Pelog  Scales, 
518c,  (2,  5226] 

[Jehmlich,  4996,  509(2] 

[Jenkin  and  Ewing,  ii&2'.  analysis  of  vowels 
by  Phonograph,  538  to  542] 

[Jimmerthal,  502c] 

Joachim  uses  4  :  5  major  Thirds  in  melody, 
255(2.    plays  in  just  intonation,  325a 

John  XXH.,  Pope,  a-d.  1322,  forbids  use  of  lead- 
ing note,  287c 

Jones,  Sir  William,  presumes  antfa  to  have 
been  the  Indian  tonic,  ^2430,  6,  c.    see  Y6di 

[Jordans,  5066] 

[Josquin,  225(2,  296(2] 

[Jots,  cycle  of  30103,  437al 

Just  intonation  instrumentally  practicable, 
327(2.  in  singing,  422  to  428.  [extreme  close- 
ness of  its  representation  by  the  cycle  of  53, 
3296,  c]  the  protest  of  musicians  against 
it  arises  from  their  not  having  methodically 
compared  Just  and  Tempered  Intonation,428c, 
possible  in  the  orchestra,  428c.  natural,  428a. 
according  to  Delezenne,  4286.  [expressed  in 
cycle  of  1200,  440.  experimeDtal  instruments 
for  exhibiting,  App.  XX.  Seot»  F,  4666  to  483c. 
zee  contents,  4666] 

Digitized  by  V^jOOQ IC 


566 


INDEX. 


Jnstly  intoned  Harmoniums,  with  two  msnnals 
arranged  by  the  Author,  316c  to  319c.  instru- 
ments necessary  for  teaching  singing,  327a. 
instruments,  plan  for  them,  with  a  single 
manual,  421a  to  4226 

K 

Eeppler  could  not  free  himself  from  musical 
imagination,  22gd 

Kettledrums,  their  secondary  tones  noi  inves- 
tigated, 736 

Key  of  polyphonic  composition  different  for 
different  voices,  245(2.  [acoustical  effects  of 
change  of  key,  55ic2.]     Keynote,  see  Tonic 

Keys,  have  they  special  characters?  310c. 
[their  presumed  characters,  550c] 

[Khorassan,  Tambour  of,  its  scale,  after  Prof. 
Land,  517&] 

Kiesewetter,  R.  G.,  236^2,  *28i&,  c,  282^' 

Kirchengesang,  or  congregational  singing, 
•287^' 

Eircher,  Athanasius,  finds  both  macrocosm 
and  microcosm  musical,  2290 

[Kirkman's  harpsichord,  1773,  its  striking- 
place,  7  yd] 

^mberger  tunes  Bach's  major  Thirds  sharp, 
321C 

[Kirsten,  5096] 

Eissar,  five-string  lyre  of  North  Africa,  penta- 
tonic  according  to  Villoteau,  257c 

[Klang,  24c] 

Koenig,  B.  [when  tuning-forks  have  no 
partials,  55(2',  io6df.  on  vowel  resonance, 
♦109c',  *iogd\  i22d',  on  influence  of  dif- 
ference of  phase  on  quality  of  tone,  I26<2.] 
with  short  sounding-rods  has  shewn  that 
tones  with  4000  to  40000  vibrations  in  a 
second  can  be  heard,  151c,  iS2d,  15912,  d\ 
1 67(2.  recent  work  on  combinational  tones 
and  beats,  I52(2.]  experiments  on  forks  with 
sliding  weights,  his  results,  159&,  [i  59(21, 176c. 
makes  resonators,  372(2.  his  manometric 
flames,  374(]^,  5.  [his  clock  method  of  deter- 
mining pitch,  442c.  his  tuning-fork  tonome- 
ters, price  of  various  kinds,  446a,  &,  494(2'. 
on  beats  and  combinational  tones,  527  to  538] 

Kosegarten,  J.  G.  L.,  282(2' 

[Krebs,  510a] 

[Eummer,  509&] 

[Kfitzing,  C,  1884,  gives  ^  length  as  suitable 
striking-place,  but  had  met  with  |,  *77c'J 


[Land,  Prof.  J.  P.  N.,  Gamme  Arabe,  ♦28o<j', 
*28ic.  his  account  of  the  Arabic  scales,  284 
note.  Al  Farabi's  Greek  Scales,  515c.  Arabic 
and  Persian,  5 1  $d.  Ditto  Medieval  and  Ancient 
Lute,  5 1 66.  Tambours  of  Khorassan  and 
Bagdad,  5176,  c,  5236^ 

Larynx,  action  of,  98a 

Later  Greek  Scales,  270a,  6,  e 

Leading  note,  conception  of,  2856,  between 
Seventh  and  Octave,  not  between  Third 
and  Fourth,  286a,  but  between  Second  and 
Third  in  the  minor  mode,  2866.  not  marked, 
but  sung,  even  in  Protestant  churches,  to 
1 6th  and  17th  centuries,  287^.  found  in 
Cantus  firmuSf  2870.  not  sung  by  Esthonians 
even  when  played  by  the  organ,  288a.  exists 
only  in  two  tonal  modes,  Greek  Lydian  and 
Hypolydian,  288a.  causes  alteration  of 
Greek  Phry^an  mode  to  the  ascending  minor 
scale,  and  Greek  Eolic  to  instrumental  minor 


scale,  2886.   its  general  introduction  leads  to 

development  of  feeling  for  the  tonic,  2886,  c. 

effect  of  excessively  sharpening,  315a 
Leaps,  when  they  are  not  advisable,  3556 
[Lehnert,  509c] 
[Leibner,  508c] 
Leibnitz,  1646- 17 16,  perdpirt  and  appereipirt 

»  synthetically  and  analytically  perceived, 

62(2,  [d'] 
[Lemmens,  jMreferred  false  intervals  as  a  child, 

F6tis's  story,  zSod'] 
[Lemoine,  $oSb] 
[Lewis,  5<i6c,  507a] 
Lichaon  of  Samos,  266e 
Light  and  Sound,  analogies  of  their  oompass, 

186.     [light  extends  over  an  Octave  and  a 

Fourth,  18c',  (21 
[Linear  temperaments,  433a] 
Lips,  as  membraneous  reeds,  97c 
[Lissajous,  496(2',  and  Ferrand,  508c] 
[Listen,  Rev.  Henry,  his  organ,  4736] 
Liturgy,  Roman,  its  singing,  239a 
Locrian  (Hypodoric)  Scale,  267^ 
Low  tones,  see  Deep  tones 
[Lupot  (France,  1 750-1820),  resonanoe  ol  his 

violins,  Sjc] 

[Lushington,  V.,  and  daughter,  assist  in  find- 
ing resonance  of  violins,  Syd] 
[Lute  stop  of  harpsichord  in  i8th  century,  77<2] 
Luther,  his  feelings  on  music,  246c 
Lydian  Scale,  Glarean's,  24$d 
Lydian  Scales,  Greek,  267c 

M 

[Macfarren,  Sir  G.,  sees  right  (just  intonation) 
through  wrong  (tempered  intonation),  346^^. 
writes  sight  test  for  Tonic  Sol-fa  FeBtival, 
4270'.  on  character  of  keys,  550c,  standard 
pitch,  555c] 

Mach  and  J.  Hervert's  experiment  with  gas 
flames  before  the  end  of  open  flue-pipes,  936, 
♦93(2' 

[Macleod,  Prof.  H.,  and  Lieut.  Clark,  their  opti- 
cal method  of  finding  pitch,  ^4426,  494(2^ 

Madrigals,  244(2 

[MahiUon,  V.,  on  Boehm's  flutes,  *io3(2',  yxid', 
504(2] 

Major  chords  used  as  a  close  to  minor  modes, 
296(2,  297a 

Major  or  Ionic  mode,  its  harmony  well  de- 
veloped in  1 6th  century,  246(2.  gives  full  ex- 
pression to  tonality,  293(7.  its  harmonic 
superiority,  together  with  the  minor  mode, 
298c.    considered,  302 

Major  scale,  ascending,  2746 

Major  Seventh,  337a.  its  chord  in  the  direct 
syBtem,  349c 

Major  Sixth,  a  medial  consonance,  194c 

Major  Tenth  better  than  major  Third,  1956 

Major  Tetrads,  their  moet  perfect  poeitioDa, 

223(J 

Major  Third  a  medial  consonance,  194c  not 
easily  distinguished  by  differential  tones, 
200c,  [(2'].  just,  with  the  ratio  4  :  5  indnded 
by  Didymus  and  Ptolenueus  in  the  syntono- 
diatonic  mode,  but  not  recognised  as  a  con- 
sonance, 2280.  that  it  was  not  considered  a 
consonance  was  due  to  the  tonal  systems 
used,  228(2.  not  clearly  defined  and  doobtfol 
in  pure  melody,  2$$d  [indicated  by  -h  after 
276(2.]    just,  3346 

Major  Thirteenth  worse  than  major  Sixth, 

Major  triads,  their  combinational  tones  gene- 
Digitized  by  V^jOOQIC 


INDEX. 


567 


rally  saiUble  to  the  ehord,  215&.  their  most 
X)erfect  positions,  219c.  their  less  perfect 
positions,  220c 

JlialliuSt  see  Ear 

Marloye  makes  pipe  with  additional  piece  a 
half  length  of  wave,  for  experiment  on  plane 
of  reflexion,  gid  [explanations,  gidf,    504£2, 

508a,  5  IOC] 

Marparg  quotes  Eimberger  on  equal  tempera- 
ment, 32 ic,  [494^',  5106] 

[Mason  and  Hemlin,  511a] 

Maiheson,  his  Critica  Mtaica,  1752,  *32ie. 
says  Silbermann*s  unequal  temperament  is 
best  for  organs,  323c,  [So^d] 

[Maxwell,  Clarke,  fundamental  oolours,  64/f] 

Mayer,  Prof.  A.,  observations  on  the  duration 
of  sound  and  numbers  of  audible  beats,  417c 
and  note,  [his  eleotrographio  me^od  of  de- 
termining pitoh,  4426.  his  lecture-room  ana- 
lysis of  a  reed-tone,  549c.  his  harmonic 
curves,  5506] 

[Mazingue,  5086] 

[Mazzini  (Brescia,  1 560-1640),  resonance  of  his 
violins,  87c] 

[Meantone  Temperament,  433^.  its  history, 
546e.  used  in  1880,  in  Spain,  in  Greene's 
organs,  Ac,  549cT 

Meatus  auditorius,  130a 

Mechanical  problem  of  transference  of  the 
vibrations  of  air  to  labyrinth  of  ear,  134^2 

[Mediant  Duodene,  462a] 

f Meeson,  his  elliptical  tension  bars,  552rf] 

Melodic  relationship,  original  development  of 
the  feeling  for,  3686 

Melodies  in  modem  music  supposed  to  arise 
from  harmonies,  2536 

Melody  is  not  resolved  harmony,  289a.  in 
simple  tones,  how  appreciated,  2896  to  290&. 
expresses  motion  appreciable  by  immediate 
perception,  252a.  goes  far  beyond  an  imita- 
tion of  nature,  371a.  does  not  arise  from 
distinctive  cries,  3716 

Membranes,  circular,  their  sympathetic  re- 
sonance, 40c  to  41  c 

Membranes,  stretched,  their  inharmonic  proper 
tones,  736 

Membranous  tongues,  how  to  make,  97a. 
[reeds,  5546] 

[Mental  effects  of  each  degree  of  the  scale, 
279^  note] 

Mental  tune  «  OemilthsBiimmung,  2$od 

Mercadier,  see  Ck>rnu,  *Z2Sd 

Mercator*s  cycle  of  53,  328c,  [436c] 

[Merkel,  io6c'.  on  whispering,  *io84'.  his 
comparison  of  all  opinions  on  vowel  reso- 
nance with  comparative  table,  *  109c,  (2,  nod] 

[Mersenne,  494(2,  508a,  6,  d,  spinet,  509a.  on 
equal  temperament,  548c] 

Mesl^  middle  string,  answered  to  tonic,  242c 

Meshaqah,  ^2646',  26$d.  his  modem  Arabic 
scale  of  24  Quartertones,  2646,  285^',  5256,  d 

Metal  reeds,  986 

Metallic  quality  of  tone,  71 6 

[Meyerbeer,  499c] 

Middle-tone,  see  MesH 

[Millimetres,  their  relation  to  inches,  42^ 

Minor  chord,  its  treatment  by  older  com- 
posers, avoided  at  close,  217a.  its  use  at  the 
dose  marks  the  period  of  modem  music,  365a. 
its  root  or  fundamental  bass  ambiguous,  291c. 
how  avoided,  295a.  on  the  second,  a  real 
modulation  into  the  subdominant,  299a 

Minor-Major  Mode,  Hauptmann*s  name,  and 
its  chain  of  chords,  305& 

Minor  mode  arises  fi*om  fusion  of  Doric,  Eolic, 


and  Phrygian  in  Monteverde's  time,  248^2. 
does  not  give  full  expression  to  tonality,  2946. 
formerly  closed  with  major  chords,  296^, 
297a.  the  harmonic  superiority  of  this  and 
the  major  mode,  298c 

Minor  scale,  ascending  and  descending  forms, 
2746,  c.  instrumental  form,  2886.  the  effect 
on  its  chords  of  using  the  major  Seventh,  299c 

Minor  Seventh  of  scale  always  replaceable  by 
major  Seventh,  2996,  c.  the  effect  of  this  on 
the  chords  of  minor  scale,  299c.  acuter  or 
^der,  336c,  d,  chord  of  the,  in  the  direct 
system,  349c 

Minor  Sixth  an  imperfect  consonance,  194^2. 
[its  partials  compared  with  those  of  subminor 
Seventh,  1956] 

[Minor  Submediant  Duodene,  4626] 

Minor  system  inferior  to  major  in  harmonious- 
ness,  301 6,  c.  this  nowise  depreciates  its 
value,  302a.  its  capabilities  in  consequence, 
302a 

Minor  Tenth,  much  worse  than  minor  Third, 
196a 

Minor  Tetrads,  their  best  positions,  2246 

Minor  Third,  an  imperfect  consonance,  I94<2. 
[indicated  by— from  276^.]  Pythagorean,  3346* 
just,  3346 

Minor  Thirteenth,  much  worse  than  minor 
Sixth,  196a 

Minor  triad,  false,  340a.  [examples  analysed, 
340d.]  not  so  harmonious  as  major  triads, 
214c.  their  combinational  tones  do  not  belong 
to  the  harmony,  216a.  effect  of  such  tones, 
2 1 6c.  their  most  perfect  and  less  perfect 
positions,  22i5,  c 

Mixolydian  scale,  Greek,  267^.  Glarean's, 
245(2.  considered  as  mode  of  the  Fourth,  302^2 

Mode  of  Fourth  ascending  form,  275a.  of 
minor  Seventh  descending  form,  275a,  ascend- 
ing form,  2756.  of  minor  Sixth  descending 
form,  2756.  its  chain  of  chords,  305c,  d.  its 
relation  to  the  major  scale,  306&.  traces  of 
its  use  by  Handel,  307a,  &.  by  Bach,  307c. 
by  Mozart,  308a,  6.     by  Beethoven,  30S6 

[Modelet  =  ragiini,  525c^ 

[Modem  Greek  scales,  556^'] 

Modes,  ecclesiastical,  their  system  differed 
greatly  from  modem  keys,  247^2.  Professor 
Helmholtz's  names  for,  compared  with  an- 
cient Greek  and  ecclesiastical,  269a.  an- 
cient Greek,  tabulated  with  the  same  initial 
or  tonic,  269a.  the  five  with  variable  Seconds 
or  Sevenths,  in  the  new  notation,  277a'.  see 
Tonal  Modes 

MddVfilus  of  cochlea,  137& 

Modulation  in  the  modem  sense,  unknown  to 
polyphonic  music,  246a.  rules  for,  328a.  [into 
the  Dominant  Duodene,  46i(2.  into  the  sub- 
domins^,  mediant,  minor  submediant,  rela- 
tive and  correlative  Duodenes,  462] 

Mongolian  pentatonic  scales,  257c 

Monochord,  14^.  [liable  to  error,  15a],  746. 
used  to  train  singers,  326a.  [experiments 
with,  by  Messrs.  Hipkins  and  Hartan,  with 
Translator,  442a] 

[Monneron,  497c] 

Monteverde,  Claudio,  a.d.  i  565-1649,  invents 
solo  songs  with  airs,  248c.  first  composer  who 
used  chords  of  the  dominant  Seventh  without 
preparation,  24842,  296a 

[Montal,  4986] 

[Moore  A  Moore,  London,  makers  of  Harmo- 
nioal,  1 76] 

Moore,  H.  Keatley,  ytjd,  31  td,  [says  Handel 
took  a  chorus  from  Carissimi,  307(2.  explains 


Digitized  by 


^.joogle 


568 


INDEX. 


action  of  short  keys  of  piano  on  the  character 
of  the  keys,  Slid] 

Motets,  245a 

Motion  measures  power  in  the  inorganic  world, 
26.    periodic,  illustrated,  86.    undulatory,  ga 

Movements,  musical,  chiefly  depend  on  psycho- 
logical action,  2C 

Mozart  [uses  pianofortes  by  Stein,  77^].  some- 
times concludes  a  minor  piece  with  a  major 
chord,  2176.  his  use  of  chords  in  his  Ave 
verum  cormu,  225a.  his  use  of  closing  minor 
chord,  29&2,  2970.  his  Bequiem^  2976.  his 
use  of  the  mode  of  the  minor  Sixth,  308a,  6. 
his  Proiegga  il  giusto  cielo  seldom  sung  satis- 
factorily, 326a.  lived  at  the  commencement 
of  equal  temperament,  327c,  [4966,  548<] 

Mueller,  Johannes  (1801-58),  starts  physio- 
logical acoustics,  46.  his  form  of  membra- 
nous tongues,  976.  his  theory  of  the  specific 
energies  of  sense,  148c 

Music,  most  closely  related  to  sensation  of  all 
arts,  2d.  depends  for  material  on  sensation 
of  tone,  3a.  had  to  shape  and  select  its  ma- 
terials, 250a.  expresses  states  of  sensitive- 
ness, 250c,  25  ic 

Musical  quality  of  tone,  676 

Musical  tones,  7c,  106,  c,  without  opper 
partials,  69c.  with  inharmonic  upper  par- 
tials,  70a.  of  strings,  74a.  of  bowed  instru- 
ments, 80c.  of  reed  pipes,  95a.  on  compound 
tones  are  chords  of  partials  and  hence  repre- 
sented by  chords,  3096 

[Musician's  cycle  of  55,  436(2] 

Mysis  (or  opossum  shrimp)  hears  when  oto- 
liths are  extirpated,  149(2.  tuning  of  its  audi- 
tory hairs,  150a 


[Naeke,  Herr  and  Frau,  494^'] 
[Natural,  the  symbol  Q ,  whence  derived,  3X2(2] 
*  Naturalness '  of  the  major  chord,  according 
to  Rameau  and  d*Alembert,  and  insufficiency 
of  such  assumption,  2326,  c 
Natural  Seventh    (see  Subminor  Seventh) 
Nasals,  M,  N,  N',  their  humming  effect,  have 
peculiarities  of  U,  117a 
Naumann,  G.  E.,  276(2.    defends  Pythagorean 
intonation,  2^40,  *3i4d',  328(2 
Neidhard,  equal  temperament,  170&,  *32ic,  df 
Nerve  force  has  only  quantitative,  not  qualita- 
tive difFerenoes,  the  different  results  depend  on 
the  different  terminals,  149c 
Netherland  system,  its  harsh  polyphony,  2256 
Neumann,  Clem.,  simpler  way  of  observing 
vibrational  form  of  violin  string  by  a  grating, 
♦83(2' 

[Neutral  Intervals,  525a.    Third,  Prof.  Land's 
name  for  ZalzaPs  Third,  2Sid] 
New  Caledonia,  pentatonic  scales,  257c 
New  Guinea  Papuas,  pentatonic  scales,  2570 
Newton   (1642-.1727),  on  laws  of  motion  of 
strings,  15a 

[Nichol's  Germania  Orchestra,  511 6] 
Nicomachus,  his  comparison  of  the  seven  tones 
to  the  seven  heavenly  bodies,  241a,  [c\  d'].  on 
the  old  tuning  of  the  lyre,  255c,  257(2',  266c 
Nodes  of  strings,  how  to  find  ^ose  on  a  piano- 
forte, 47(2,  5(3(2,  and  how  to  touch,  51a.    par- 
tials of  strings  which  have  no  node  in  a  certain 
spot  are  silenced  by  touching  the  string  at 
the  node,  52(2.   ^exceptions,  78(2,  5406] 
Noise-  defined,  7c,  <2,  "80.    accompanying  in- 
strumental   notes,   67c.    perception    of,    by 
cochlea,  i5€)^ 


[Northcote,  Miss,  blind  organist  of  Oen.  P. 

Thompson's  Enharmonic  organ,  4230^ 
Note,  musical,  its  construction,  5c.    lised  for 

any  musical  tone,  24a,  d 
Notation,  new,  for  distinguishing  the  relatioDS 

of  Fifths  from  those  of  major  Thirds,  276a  to 

2786  [substitute  here  used,  277c] 
Numbers,  what  have  the  ratios  of  the  first  six 

to  do  with  music  ?  2a,    see  Pythagoras 


[Oberzahn,  25(2'] 

Oboe,  its  tongues  or  reeds,  966,  554c.  has  a 
conical  tube,  and  produces  all  haxmonioa,  99a 

Observation,  personal,  better  than  best  de- 
scription, 6a 

Octave,  a  collection  of  eight  notes  (uBually 
printed  with  a  small  letter),  onacoenied,  or 
4-foot,  15(2.  once  accented  or  2-foot,  and 
twice  accented,  or  i-foot,  16a.  great,  or  8- 
foot,  and  contra,  or  16-foot,  and  32-foot,  i6b 

Octave.  [Octave  meaning  interval  (usually 
printed  with  a  capital  letter),  and  octave, 
meaning  set  of  notes  (usually  printed  with  a 
small  letter),  I3(2.]  easy  to  maike  the  mistake 
of,  as  Tartini,  Henrici,  and  others,  62a. 
gives  no  beats  except  those  from  partials  in 
a  single  compound,  iSyd,  same  for  double 
Octave,  1 87(2.  greatly  distorts  adjacent  con- 
sonances, 1 88a.  an  absolute  consonance, 
1946.  of  simple  tones,  distinguished  by  the 
first  differential  tone,  1990.  repetition  in  it 
presents  nothing  but  what  has  already  been 
heard,  2$yL  not  allowed  in  composition, 
359c.  why  its  key  is  identified  with  that  of 
the  prime,  3296 

[Octave  divisions,  their  possible  origin,  522(2] 

Oettingen,  A.  von,  his  notation  for  relations  of 
Thirds  and  Fifths,  277c.  its  modification  by 
Translator  here  used,  277c  note,  report  00 
Esthonian  treatment  of  leading  note  in  minor 
scales,    ^287(2.    his   minor   system,    *3o8c2, 

3656 

Ohm,  G.  S.,  1 787-1 854,  his  law,  336,  c,  76*. 
completion  of  its  proof,  56c.  Seebeck's  ob- 
jections to  it,  *sSd,  his  experiment  with  a 
violin  to  shew  fusion  of  note  and  Octave,  606. 
better  form  with  bottles  blown  by  stream  of 
air  over  mouth,  6oe 

Olivier,  ♦io8c' 

Olympos,  B.C.  660-620,  his  pentatonic  trans- 
formation of  the  Doric  scale,  258a.  his 
ancient  enharmonic  tetrachord,  262c 

Open  pipes,  see  Organ  pipes,  open 

Opera,  one  of  the  most  active  causes  of  deve- 
lopment of  harmony  in  1 7th  oentnry,  2486 

[Organ-builders'  measurement  of  octaves  by 
feet,  l6d] 

Organ  pipes,  wide  stopped,  unsnited  for  har- 
mony, delimit  consonance  imperfectly,  un- 
snited for  polyphony,  their  use,  2056.  flue, 
delimit  Octaves  and  Fifths  by  partials,  but 
require  combinational  tones  for  the  Thirds, 
205(7.  open,  good  for  harmony  and  poly- 
phony, 206a 

Oigan,  its  compound  stops  have  fewer  pipes  in 
upper  notes,  2106 

Organ  stops,  QuintaUn,  33(2.  Twelfth,  3312. 
principal  register ^  weiiigedacki^  gei^en-regiaUfi 
quintaUn^  comet,  compound,  57b.  ^usqui- 
altera^  comet  mounted,  57(2'.}  The  musician 
mnst  regard  all  tones  as  resembling  the  ecai- 
pound  ^rgan  stops^  s?^ 


Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


INDEX. 


569 


Organiun  of  Haebald,  244a 

Oscillation,  length  of,  86.    period  of,  86 

Ossicles,  see  Auditory 

Otoliths,  ear  stones,  see  Auditory 

[Ouseley,  Sir  F.  A.  Gore,  503a] 

Outwards  striking  reeds,  97c 

Oval  window  of  labyrinth,  130a,  136& 

[Overtones,  used  by  Prof.  TyndaU  for  upper 
partial  tones,  an  error  of  translation,  here 
avoided ;  the  term  should  never  be  used  for 
partials  in  genecal,  2$d'] 

[Overtooth,  254'J 


[Paoifio,  South,  Balafong,  518c,  ^226] 
Palestrina,  a.i>.  i524-*I594,  under  Pius  IV., 
pupil  of  Goudimel,  2476.  carries  out  sim- 
pli£oation  of  church  music,  247a.  his  use 
of  chords,  especially  in  the  opening  of  his 
Stabat   tnateTt    2256,  247c,  2486,    296a,  c. 

[Paris,  Aim6  and  Kanine,  pupils  of  Galin, 
425c'.    Alms's  bridge  tones,  426&J 

Partial  tones  in  general,  and  upper  partial 
tones  in  particular,  how  distinguished,  22a. 
in  musical  notes,  22c.  [partialt&ne^pta- 
tial  tones,  24c\]  partials,  contraction  for 
partial  tones,  240'.  no  illusion  of  the  ear 
any  more  than  prismatic  colours  are  of  the 
eye,  48c.  those  unevenly  numbered  are 
easier  to  observe,  49c.  methods  for  observ- 
ing by  ear,  50c.  on  piano,  50c.  on  strings 
generally,  50c  to  51a.  modes  of  observing  on 
human  voice,  516.  [high  upper,  their  exist- 
ence proved  by  beats  with  forks  by  Trans- 
lator, 56d'.]  partials  fuse  into  a  compound 
tone,  shewn  by  experiments.  Ohm's  with  a 
violin.  Prof.  Helmholtz's  with  bottles,  606,  c. 
[Translator's  with  tuning-forks  and  resonant 
jars,  6 id.]  upper,  their  influence  on  quality 
of  tone,  62a.  inharmonic  upper,  706. 
favoured  on  a  piano  whose  period  is  nearly 
twice  the  duration  of  stroke,  76a.  of  a 
string-tone  disappear  which  have  a  node  at 
point  excited,  76c,  77a.  [not  always  when 
struck  by  a  pianoforte  hammer,  y6dt  78c, 
54.66.]  ^ewn  by  flames  seen  in  a  revolving 
mirror,  Koenig*B  manometric  flames,  374a, 
6.  [of  a  pianoforte  string  struck  at  one- 
eightii  its  length,  545c.]  upper,  of  human 
voice,  difficult  to  recognise  but  heard  by 
Kameau  and  Seller,  1046.  upper,  perceived 
synthetically,  even  when  not  anfUytically, 
656.  by  properly  directed  attention  they  may 
be  observed  analytically,  65c.  at  any  rate 
they  effect  an  alteration  of  quality  of  tone,  65c 

Passing  notes,  353a 

[Patna,  Balafong  from,  518a,  521^2'] 

[Pauer,  Ernst,  on  presumed  character  of  keys, 
S5od\  SSia] 

Paul,  O.,  considers  that  Hncbald  invented 
the  principle  of  imitation,  ^2440 

[Pedals  -  footkeys,  50^1 

[Pellisov,  see  Schafhautl,  lo^d] 

IPelog  scales,  51 8(2,  526a] 

Pendular  or  simple  vibrations,  23a.  their 
law  and  form,  230^ 

Pendulum,  its  periodic  motion,  190.  how  set 
swinging  by  the  hand,  by  periodically  moving 
the  point  of  suspension,  376.  to  shew  vibration 

of  membrane  due  to  that  of  air  in  bottle,  42a 

Pentatonic  scales  in  China,  Mongolia,  Java, 
Sumatra,  Hudson's  Bay,  New  Guinea,  New 
Caledonia,  and  among  Fullah  negroes,  257c. 


five  varieties,  259a,  6,  e.    [259^.    numerous 
other,     5186.      independent    of    heptatonic 
scales,  525^.    do  not  arise  from  inability  to 
appreciate  Semitones,  5266] 
[Peppercorn,  496^,  497a,  549a] 
Perception,  synthetical  and  analytical,  62d 
Peri,  Giacomo,   in    1600   invents   recitative, 
2456,  248c 
Periodic  motions  of  pendulum,  19c.    of  water- 
wheel  hammer,  19c.    of  ball  struck  up  when 
falling,  igd 

Periods  of  musical  composition,  three,  236^ 
Persian,  see  Arabic 

Persian  music  later  develops   12  Semitones, 
285a.    Eiesewetter's  hypothesis,  285a  [d] 
[Phonograph,  Edison's,  used  to  analyse  vowel 
sounds,  by  Messrs.  Jenkin  A  Ewing,  539a] 
Phase,  difference  of,  346.    its  effects  on  forms 
of  vibrational  curves,  iigd.     on  quality  of 
tone,  1 20c.    in  compounded  simple  tones  does 
not  affect  quality  of  tone,  124c  to  127c.    as 
seen  in  the  vibration  microscope,  I26(2,  127a. 
[its  influence  on  quality  of  tone  according  to 
Koenig,  537a] 
Philolaus,  257^ 

Phrynis,  victor  at  Panathenaic  competitions, 
t      adds  a  ninth  string  to  his  lyre,  269c 
I    Phrygian  scale,  267c.      Greek » mode  of  the 
I     minor  Seventh,  303c.    Glarean's,  245^2,  305c 
Pianoforte,  echoes  vowels,  61  c  [izgd,]    strings, 
where  struck,  776  [77c.]    takes  the  first  place 
among  instruments  with  struck  strings,  2o8d. 
the  quality  of  its  chords  arising  from  the 
quality  of  its  tones,  2090,  c,  d,    bears  disso- 
nances well,  2096.    its  strings,  mathematical 
investigation  of  their  vibrational  forms,  380a. 
[its  string  struck  at  one-eighth  its  length,  has 
the  8th  harmonic  and  partial,  545c.]    see  also 
Hammer 

[Pichler,  tuner  at  Berlin  opera,  $09(2] 
Pipes,  their  theory,  388-397.  theory  of  blow- 
ing them,  mathematically  treated,  390  to 
3966.  conical,  calculated  series  of  their  tones, 
393c.  [with  remarks  note  *  and  394/2  note  *,] 
see  also  Organ  pipes 

Pitch,  IOC.  number  defined,  iia  [iidJ]  de- 
pends only  on  the  number  of  vibrations  in 
a  second,  13c.  numbers  of  just  musical 
scale,  how  calculated,  156,  e.  Scheibler's 
1 6c.  French,  i6<2.  numbers  of  the  just 
musical  scale,  tabulated  to  a'440,  17a.  of  a 
compound  tone,  is  the  pitch  of  its  prime, 
24a.  its  definite  appreciation  begins  at  40 
vib.  177a.  alters  by  definite  intervals  and 
why,  2506,  2526,  d.  of  tonic  undetermined, 
depending  on  compass  of  voice  or  instrument, 
310a.  [numbers,  how  to  determine,  App.  XX. 
sect.  B,  see  contents,  4416.  musical,  de- 
fined, 4946.  its  history.  495  to  513,  sect.  H. 
see  contents,  493(2.  Church,  lowest,  495a. 
low,  4956.  medium,  499a.  high,  503a. 
highest,  503(2.  extreme,  524.  chamber  low, 
4950.  highest,  504a.  mean  of  Europe  for 
two  centuries,  495(2,  497(2.  compromise, 
497(2.  modem  orc^estrid,  499a.  when  it 
began  to  rise,  512c] 

[Pitman,  organist  at  Covent  Garden  Theatre, 
Sood] 

[Pitt-Rivers,  Gen.,  his  balafong,  5186,  522a] 
Pius  IV.,  Pope,  AJ).  1559-1565,  orders  simpli- 
fication of  church  music,  247a 
Pizzicato  of  violin  more  piercing  than  piano 
tones,  67a,  746 

Plagal  scales,  Glarean's  six,  245c.  the  fourth, 
267(»,  271(2 


Digitized  by  V^jOOQlC 


570 


INDEX. 


Plastio  arts  address  the  eye  as  poetry  does  the 
ear,  2d 

Playford,  •atoi' 

Plectrum,  74c 

Plucking  removes  the  whole  string  from  its 
position  of  rest,  74^.  the  intensity  of  prime 
is  greater  than  that  of  any  partial,  75a 

Plutarch  on  the  Scale,  2626.  thinks  the  later 
Greeks  had  a  preference  for  the  surviving 
archaic  intervals,  *2'-  $d' 

Poetry,  its  aim  and  means  mainly  psychical,  2d 

[Pole,  Dr.  W.,  on  characters  of  keys,  550c] 

Politzer,  experiment  on  the  round  window, 
1366.  produces  drawings  of  beats  by  attach- 
ing a  style  to  the  columella  (auditory  ossicle) 
of  a  duck,  1666 

Polyphonic  music  generated  by  discant,  2446 
to  246.  siren,  description  of  mechanism  for 
opening  the  several  series  of  holes  in  it,  413c 

Polyphonic  siren,  see  Siren,  i>olyphonic 

[Pomeranian  (Blucher)  band,  its  very  high 
pitch,  555(f| 

Poole,  H.  W.  [I95<2',  2I&2,  222^',  22Sd,  323d', 
329<i'.  his  dichordal  scale,  344c.]  his  Euhar- 
monic  Organ,  *423a,[c.  his  proposed  finger- 
board, and  theories  of  Seventh  Harmonic, 
474a  to  4/ 9a.  writes  to  Translator  from 
Mexico  about  his  latest  fingerboards,  478^] 

Position  of  stroke  that  excites  a  string,  76c 

Positions  of  a  chord  not  hitherto  regarded  in 
musical  theory,  224c 

Poverty  of  tone,  in  what  it  consists,  756 

PrsBtorius  mentions  a  cymbalum  with  19  digi- 
tals to  the  octave,  320c,  320(2'.  on  '  wolves,' 
321a.  [on  early  pitch,  '^4946,  494i,  49  ;a, 
5096,  c] 

[Preece  and  Stroh  on  quantity  of  sound,  *75i', 
124^.    synthetical  production  of  vowels,  542^ 

Preparation  of  dissonant  notes,  353d 

Preyer,  W.,  high  pitch  from  forks,  i8a  [♦iS^T], 
55(2'].  on  distinguishable  intervals,  147c  [*d], 
by  Appunn's  tuning-forks  has  shewn  that 
tones  with  4000  to  40000  vib.  in  a  second  are 
audible,  151c,  167J.  [his  recent  work  on 
combinational  tones  and  beats,  15 2d,  '153c, 
156^,  167^,  ♦i76d,  d\  1776,  d',  202d,  204^, 
205^.  his  experiments  with  two  forks  of  137 
and  18*6  vib.,  177^'.]  finds  the  difference 
between  tones  of  tuning-forks  and  reeds  dis- 
appears at  4224  vib.,  179c.  his  experiments 
with  Appunn's  weighted  reeds,  l^(>d,  says 
as  low  as  15  vib.  may  be  heard,  176^.  Prof, 
fielmholtz  inclined  to  think  the  tongues  may 
have  given  double  their  nominal  pitch,  176c'. 
[the  Translator's  experiments  to  determine 
the  real  pitch  of  these  reeds,  176^'.  226^, 
229^',  23 id',  on  beats  and  combinational 
tones,  528  to  538J 

Prime  of  a  compound  tone  defined,  22a 

Principal-stimmen  of  organs,  q3c 

[Principal  work  of  organs,  93d'] 

[Proch,  504d] 

Ihrogression  of  Chords  of  the  Seventh,  357d  to 
358d.  by  Fifths,  355d.  by  Thirds,  3566.  its 
laws  are  subject  to  many  exceptions,  356d 

Protestant  congregational  singing,  its  musical 
results,  246c 

[Provost-Ponsin,  Mme.,  pronounces  last  sylla- 
ble of  hachis  without  voice,  68d] 

Proximity  in  the  scale,  a  new  point  of  connec- 
tion between  tones,  287a 

Ptolemy  included  the  major  Third  4  :  5  in  the 
syntono-diatonic  mode,  22SC.  his  tuning  of 
the  equal  diatonic  tetrachord,  2646 

[Pythagorean   minor    Thirds   indicated   typo- 


graphically by  I  ,  276d.  intonation  in  15th 
century,  313d.  temperament,  433aJ 
Pythagoras  (fl.  b.c.  540-510),  his  discorety  of 
law  of  consonance  for  strings,  id.  extended 
to  pitch  numbers,  id.  the  physiological 
reason  of  his  numerical  law,  56.  how  he  dis- 
covered his  law  of  intervals,  I4d.  was  the 
sole  hearer  of  the  harmony  of  the  spberee, 
229c.  his  enigma,  'why  consonance  is  de- 
termined by  ratios  of  small  whole  numbers,' 
solved  by  discovery  of  partials,  229a,  249a. 
his  tetrachord,  2636.  first  used  8  degrees  to 
the  scale  forming  an  octave,  2666.  his  dis- 
junct tetrachords,  266c.  constructed  his  scales 
from  a  series  of  seven  Fifths,  278d.  'or 
rather  Fourths,  2796,  c,  note  *.]  his  system 
used  as  foundation  of  temperament,  322a 


Quality  of  tone,  loe,  18&.  defined,  19a,  depends 
on  form  of  vibration,  19&,  2id.  its  concep- 
tion, 65d.  must  not  be  credited  with  the 
noises  of  attack  and  release  of  tones,  66a,  nor 
with  rapidity  of  dying  away,  66c.  its  strict 
meaning,  676.  musical,  69a.  of  reed  pipes, 
loia,  I02d.  [of  vowels  depends  not  on  the 
absolute  pitch,  but  on  relative  force  of  upper 
partials,  113d'.]  recapitulation  of  results  of 
Chap,  v.,  II 8d  to  119c.  apprehension  of, 
I  I9d,  1286.  independent  of  difference  of  phase 
in  the  partials  combined,  124c  to  127c.  appa- 
rent exceptions,  127c,  [which  are  thought  to 
be  real  by  Koenig,  I26d,  537a] 

Quanten,  E.  von,  his  objections  to  Helmholtz's 
vowel  theory,  11 5d 

Quartertones,  Arabic  scale  of,  2646.  [noted  by 
q  a  turned  b,  264d'.    temperament,  525a] 

Quartett  playing,  why  it  often  sounds  Ul,  324d. 
singing  of  amateurs  often  just,  3266 

Quincke,  *37 7d 

Quintatsn  (quintam  tenentes)  organ  stop,  946 


B 


B  letter,  its  beats,  67d,  168& 

[Bab4b,  scale  after  Prof.  Land,  517c] 

[Baffles,  526a] 

[Baginl,  or  modelets,  525d] 

[B&m  P41  Singh,  B&ja,  his  Quartertones,  265^. 
his  scales,  5i7d] 

Bameau,  1685-1764,  hears  upper  partials  of 
human  voice  without  apparatus,  ^516,  1046. 
his  theory  of  consonance,  '232a  to  2336.  com- 
plete expression  not  given  to  the  new  harmonic 
view  till  his  time,  2496.  his  assumption  of  an 
*  understood '  fundamental  bass,  253c.  his 
chord  of  the  great  or  added  Sixth,  294d.  his 
fundamental  bass,  radical  tone  or  root,  309c. 
the  tuning  he  defended  in  1726, 321a.  subee- 
quently  proposes  equal  temperament,  321^, 
32 ic,  35 id.  his  law  of  motion  of  the  funda- 
mental bass  by  Fifths  or  Thirds,  355d.  when 
he  allows  a  diatonic  progression  of  the  funda- 
mental bass,  3566 

Bational  construction  of  scales,  272c  to  2756. 
differs  materially  from  the  Pythagorean,  278d 

[Bayleigh,  Lord,  distance  of  plane  of  reflexion 
from  end  of  flue-pipe,  9  id.  his  dock  method 
of  determining  pitch,  442d.  his  harmonium 
reed  method,  4436] 

Becitative,  invented  in  1600,  by  Peri  A  Caceini, 
248c 


Digitized  by  V^OOQIC 


INDEX. 


571 


Becitation»  musioal,  239a 

BeedB  or  vibrators  of  harmoniumB,  how  they 
ftct,  95a,  6.  how  their  quality  of  tone  is 
modified,  95c.  on  organs  and  harmoniums 
for  one  note  each,  980.  on  wooden  wind  in- 
struments one  reed  serves  for  several  notes, 
986.  of  clarinets,  966,  oboes  and  bassoons, 
97a.  striking  inwards  and  outwards,  976,  c. 
notes  proper  to  them  not  used  at  all  on 
wooden  wind  instruments,  98c.  pipes,  their 
musical  tones,  95a.  of  organs,  96a.  pipes,  their 
quality  of  tone,  loia,  I02d,  experiments  to 
shew  that  they  are  modified  by  resonance 
chambers,  102&.  with  cylindrical  pipes,  391 6. 
striking  inwards  and  outwards,  391^.  metal, 
392a.  with  conical  pipes,  392^.  pipes,  theory  of 
blowing  them,  mathematically  treated,  39odt 
394a.  [action  of,  by  Hermann  Smith,  5536 
to  555c'.  their  kinds,  553c.  for  clarinets, 
553c.  for  Hichi-rikiy  SSScf.  for  bassoons  and 
oboes,  554a.  membranous,  5546.  stream, 
544c.  free  reed,  Chinese,  554^.  harmonium, 
554a'.  American  organ,  5546'.  voicing  of, 
554^2'.     suction    chambers,    5556.     drilled, 

555^1 

[Begister,  used  by  T.  Young  for  quality  of 
tone,  why  not  so  used  here,  24I/] 

[Registers  of  the  voice  in  singing,  from  Lennox 
Browne  and  Emil  Behnke,  described,  lood'  to 
loic,  d,  c'  d"] 

Beissner's  membrane,  i27d 

Belationship  of  tones  in  first  and  second  de- 
gree, 2566.  differs  with  the  quality  of  the 
tone,  256c.  due  in  melody  to  memory,  in 
harmony  to  immediate  sensation,  368^.  be- 
tween tones  more  than  two  Fifths  apart  im- 
perceptible, 2796.   of  chords,  its  degrees,  2966 

[Relative  Duodene,  4626] 

Resemblance,  unconscious  sense  of,  369c 

Resonance  of  boxes  of  violin,  violoncello,  and 
vi61a,  866  [S6d\  876  to  d'].  of  cavities  of 
mouth,  how  to  find,  I04d  to  105a.  depends 
on  vowel  uttered  independent  of  age  and  sex, 
105&.  [difficulties  of  determining  it,  105c.]  of 
cavity  of  mouth,  how  it  affects  vowels,  i  loc. 
its  influence  in  reed  pipes,  mathematically 
treated,  388  to  390a 

Res'onators  for  separating  the  musical  tones 
in  noise,  yd.  spherical  and  cylindrical,  436,  c. 
Bosanquet's,  43d'.  advantages  of  a  tuned 
aeries  of,  446.  use  in  finding  partials,  51c. 
affect  the  prime  tone  of  the  voice  as  well  as 
partials,  Uieir  effect  on  reed  pipes,  1126. 
spherical,  their  advantages,  372^2.  formula 
for  their  pitch,  373a.  of  glass,  their  dimen- 
sions, 373c.  tin  or  pasteboard  in  double 
cones,  373^.    conical,  373^ 

[Resultant  tones.  Prof.  Tyndall's  name  for 
combinational  tones,  153c] 

Results  of  the  whole  investigation,  362a  to  3666 

Retrospect  of  results  in  Parts  I.  &  II.,  226  to  :<33 

Reverted  system,  its  chords,  342c.  serve  to 
mark  the  key,  3446 

[Beyher,  S.,  on  vowel  resonance,  *io8d,  109c] 

Richness  of  tone,  in  what  it  consists,  756 

Riemann,  Hugo,  *^6sdt  *4i  ic 

[Ritchie,  E.  8.,  511a] 

Robson,  Messrs.,  built  Gen.  Perronet  Thomp- 
son's Enharmonic  Organ,  423a 

[Rockstro,  W.  S.,  doubts  whether  the  authentic 
scales  are  rightly  attributed  to  Ambrose, 
242(2.  on  Ecclesiastical  scales,  266d.  on  .ne 
Hexachord,  *35id'] 

Rockstro,  R.  S.,  intonation  of  his  *  model '  flute, 
555^  to  556a 


Rods,  effect  of  their  material  on  the  quality  of 

tone,  71a 

Rohrflbte,  organ  stop,  946,  [94^2'] 
Roman  Catholic  Church  alters  its  music,  247a 
Root  or  fundamental  bass,  294c 
[Rossetti,  Prof.  494(2] 
Roughness  of  intervals,  referred  to  the  same 

bass  note  calculated  and  constructed,  192^';, 

discussed,  193 

Round  window  of  labyrinth,  130a,  136& 
[Rudall,  lo^d] 

Rudinger  on  the  semicircular  canals,  136^2 
Rudolph  II.  of  Prague,  his  cymbalum  of  19 

tones  to  the  Octave,  320c 
[Ruggieri  (Cremona,  1668-1720),  resonance  of 

his  violins,  87c] 

[Rule  for  tuning  in  just  intonation,  493c] 
[Russia,  pitch,  5iOc2] 
[Rust,  how  it  affects  forks,  555c] 
[R,  uvular,  where  localised,  67c2J 


S 

SacculuSj  see  Ear 

St.  Paul's,  London,  tones  of  its  former  bell,  72^ 

[Salendro  Scales,  518c,  526a] 

Salicumalf  organ  stop,  94a,  [94(2] 

[Salinas,  F.,  1513-1590,  35i(2'.  his  tempera- 
ments, 5476.  completes  meantone  tempera- 
ment, 518] 

Salisbury,  Prof.,  at  Tale,  his  MS.,  281c 

[Samv&di,  or  *  minister '  note,  526c] 

Sand,  see  Auditory 

Sand  figures  on  membranes,  4i(2 

[Sarti,  S^od] 

Sauerwald,  16  id' 

[Sauveur's  cycle  of  43,  436 i',  494(2'^  509&] 

Savart  on  pitch  of  resonance  of  violin  and 
violoncello  boxes,  866,  17 $d 

Scdla,  vesdbilli  et  iympdni,  or  gallery  of  the 
vestibule  and  drum  of  the  ear,  1376 

Scales,  musical,  their  construction,  $c.  major, 
division  into  two  tetrachords,  2556.  founded  on 
relationship  of  tones,  2566.  later  Greek,  with 
conjunct  and  disjunct  tetrachord,  270a,  ^. 
[in  the  complete  notation  with  intervals  in 
cents,  274c  to  d\  harmonic  and  non-har- 
monic, 514&.  Greek,  most  ancient  form, 
5156.  later,  515c.  Al  Farabi's,  515c.  early, 
their  possible  origin,  Sizd] 

Schafhautl,  or  Pellisov,  ♦72^'.  [his  theory  of 
stopped  and  conical  pipes,  103^^ 

Scheibler,  J.  H.  (i 777-1837),  his  pitch,  i6r. 
[why  selected  by  him,  i6d\  87c,  153c,  200/, 
d',  his  tonometer,  does  not  shew  that  com- 
binational tones  of  higher  orders  existed, 
*  199(2.]  rule  for  tuning  the  Fifth  by  the 
Octave,  202d.  [not  found  by  Translator, 
202d\  his  method  of  finding  the  major 
Third  on  forks,  203^2.]  worked  out  combina- 
tional tones  for  two  simple  tones  only,  227a. 
[on  the  beauty  of  just  intonation,  423(2.  his 
tuning-fork  tonometer,  443c.  his  theoreti- 
cally perfect  method  of  tuning  pianos  and 
organs,  and  its  inconveniences,  4886.  49Ad\ 
504c,  508c,  509a,  6] 

Schiedmayer,  J.  and  P.,  made  Prof.  Helm- 
holtz's  Just  Harmonium,  Si6d* 

[Schlick,  494(2,  509c,  51 1&.  his  temperament, 
♦546(2,  5496] 

[Schmahl,  494(2',  548(2] 

[Schmidt,  Bernhardt,  called  Father  Smith, 
organ-builder,  505c] 

Schneebeli,  on  the  blade  of  air  in  flue-pipes, 
*395<2.     rhis  theory,  3966] 


Digitized  by  V^OOQIC 


572 


INDEX. 


[Schneider,  5106] 

[Schnetzler,  506a] 

[Schnitger,  uses  equal  temperament,  SA-Sd] 

[Sohreider,  496c.     and  Jordans,  505^2] 

[Schulze  of  Paulenzelle,  never  saw  a  striking 
reed  till  he  came  to  England  in  185 1,  but 
afterwards  scarcely  ased  any  other,  96c,  506&] 

Scholtze,  Max,  hairs  on  the  epithelium  am- 
pullcB,  isSd 

Science,  musical,  1 6 

Scotch  handbell  ringers,  73  J.  Scotch  pentatonic 
airs  bright,  258c.  playable  on  black  notes  of 
a  piano,  259c.  examples  of  such  airs,  2606, 
261a,  c.  [characteristics  of  Scotch  music,  ac- 
cording to  Colin  Brown,  259C2,  note  %] 

[Scotland,  bagpipe,  515^^,  519^2] 

Scott  and  Koenig*?  Phonautograph,  2od 

Scratches  of  a  bow  used  in  exciting  bodies 
with  inharmonic  upper  partials  consist  of 
those  partials,  74a.    of  a  violin,  856 

Second  of  the  Scale  undetermined,  427.  see 
Sevenths 

Sectional  Scales,  266c 

Seebeck,  A.,  his  siren,  lie.  his  objections  to 
Ohm's  law,  did  not  apply  proper  means  of 
hearing  the  upper  partials,  *58J,  considers 
Ohm's  definition  of  a  simple  tone  too  limi- 
ted, 59a.  remarks  on  Ohm's  experiment,  6 la. 
agrees  with  Ohm  that  upper  partials  are  per- 
ceived synthetically,  63a.  disputes  their 
being  perceived  analytically,  63a,  391^  note* 

Seiler,  hears  partials  of  a  watchman's  voice, 
1016 

Seiler,  Mme.,  finds  dogs  sensitive  to  &"'  of 
violin,  ii6d 

Semicircular  canals  of  the  ear,  136a 

Sensations,  of  hearing  belong  to  physiological 
acoustics,  3c.  of  sound  defined,  7c.  of 
musical  tones  and  noises,  how  generated,  8c. 
compound,  problem  of  their  analysis,  626. 
easy  when  usual,  otherwise  difiicult,  62c 

[Septimal  harmony,  464c] 

[Septendecimal  Harmony,  464c] 

Seven,  notes  characterised  by,  not  being  ad- 
mitted into  the  scale  determine  the  boundary 
between  consonance  and  dissonance,  2286 

[Seventeenth  harmonic  introduced  into  har- 
mony, 464c] 

Seventh,  see  Harmonic,  natural,  subminor, 
major,  minor,  diminished,  mode  of  the  minor 
considered,  303c.  chords  of  the,  341c.  formed 
of  two  Consonant  Trials,  341c.  formed  of 
dissonant  Triads,  342c 

Sevenths  and  Seconds  enumerated  and  con- 
sidered, 3366 

Semitones,  Chinese  view  of,  229c 

Seventh  dhninished,  3366.  chord  of  the,  upon 
the  Second  of  a  major  Scale,  347(2.  upon  the 
Second  of  a  minor  Scale,  348c.  upon  the 
Seventh  of  a  major  Scale,  3486  to  3496. 
chord  of  the  Dominant,  3476,  c.  chord  of 
the,  on  the  tonic  of  the  minor  scale,  3506 

[Seventh  harmonic  introduced  into  harmony, 
464CJ 

[Seymour,  5496] 

Sharper  or  higher  tones,  iid^ 

[Sheffield,  Sohulze's  organ  at,  96d] 

[ShMg,  Chinese,  described,  gs^ 

Shirazi,  282a 

[Siamese,  ranat,  51 86.  instruments,  music, 
scales,  and  intervals,  556a  to  5566'] 

[Side  holes  of  wind  instruments,  theory  not 
worked  out,  Blaikley,  Schafhautl,  Boehm, 
Mahillon,  io3<i,  d*] 

fSight-singing  tests  for  Tonic  Sol-fa,  AZjd"] 


Silbermann,  A.,  1678-1733,  celebrated  cngiB- 
builder,  his  unequal  temperament^  323c. 
[495&.  509c] 

[Silbermann,  G.,  495(2,  496a] 

Silence  may  result  from  two  soands,  i6oel. 
instances  of,  organ  pipes,  i6ia,  tonixig-foiks, 
161& 

Simple  musical  tones,  69c.  produced  by 
resonance  mathematically  investigated,  3776 
to  379(2.    see  also  Tone,  simple 

Simple  Vibrations,  23a 

[Smgapore,  Balafong,  518a] 

Singers,  their  form'^r  caieful  training,  326a. 
should  practise  to  justly  intoned  instrumentu, 
326(2.  their  opinion  of  Gen.  P.  Thompson*! 
organ,  427a.  take  natural  Thirds  and  Fifths, 
428a 

Singing,  [contrasted  with  speaking,  68^*.] 
voice  does  not  usually  distinguish  vowels  weD, 
1 14a.  forms  the  commencement  and  the  natu- 
ral school  of  music,  325c.  intonation  injured 
by  pianoforte  accompaniment,  3266 

Single,  distinguished  from  simple  tones,  33(f 

Siren,  116,  Seebeck's,  i  ic,Cagniard  de  la  Tour^s, 
I2C.  action  of,  136.  of  Dove,  13a,  14a.  in- 
tervals playable  on,  i62(2, 163d.  constmetion 
of,  and  beats  on,  163&,  c,  d.  polyphonic  or 
double,  its  great  use  in  determining  the  ratios 
of  consonances,  i82(2.  electro-magnetic 
driving  machine  for  the,  3726.  see  also 
Polyphonic 

Sixth  and  Fourth,  chord  of,  or  first  inversion 
of  major  and  minor  chords,  213a.  major, 
more  harmonious  than  fundamental,  and 
these  than  Sixth  and  Third,  2 14  6 

Sixth  and  Third,  or  sixth  only,  chord  of,  or 
second  inversion  of  major  and  minor  chords, 
213a.  minor  more  harmonious  than  funda- 
mental, and  these  than  Sixth  and  Fourth,  2146 

Sixth,  not  included  in  consonances  till  the^ 
13th  or  14th  century,  196c.    [Italian,  French, ' 
and  German,  461c.]    superfluous  or  extreme 
sharp,  3376.    see  also  Major  Sixth  and  Minor 
Sixth,  and  Extreme  sharp  Sixth 

Skhisma  neglected,  its  effect  in  prodadng 
identities,  281a  [465a] 

Skhismic  Relation  of  eight  Fifths  down  to  a 
major  Third,  280&,  discovered  by  Prof.  Helm- 
holtz,  3 1 6a.  [temperament,  28 1 6,  note*,  435a] 

[Smart,  Sir  G.,  507c.    his  pitch,  513a] 

[Smart,  H.,  500c] 

[Smith,  of  Bristol,  organ-builder,  4960,  5066] 

[Smith,  Christian,  organ-builder,  505(2] 

[Smith,  Eli,  S14C] 

[Smith,  Hermann,  his  account  of  the  Shteg, 
95(2.  his  account  of  Schulse's  and  Waleker's 
and  CavaiU^-Coll's  organs  with  scaroely  any 
free  reeds,  g6d,  his  account  of  the  striking 
reed,  96(2'.]  on  the  blade  of  air  in  flue-pipes, 
*395<2*  L^iB  theory,  396(2.  on  the  Action  of 
Reeds,  5536  to  SSSC^ 

[Smith,  Dr.  B.,  494^2',  510&,  5482^^ 

[Society  of  Arts,  494^ 

Solidity,  sensation  of,  analysed  by  stereoscope, 
63c 

Solo  songs  vrith  airs  invented  by  Monteverdi 
and  Viadana,  248c 

SondhausB,  his  formula  for  pitch  of  resooatonv 

3736 

Sorge,  German  organist,  1745,  discovers  com- 
binational tones,  *  1 52(2 

Sound,  velocity  of,  in  open  air,  god^  [and  in 
tubes  according  to  Mr.  D.  J.  Blaikley,  god] 

Soureck  on  the  blade  of  air  in  flue-pipet,  3951L 
this  account,  3970*] 


Digitized  by  V^OOQIC 


INDEX. 


573 


[Spain,  pitoh,  511a] 

[Speaking  contrasted  with  singing,  6Sd'.] 
▼oioe,  more  jarring  than  singing  voice,  i  ijd 

Speech,  its  natural  intonation,  238c 

Spheres,  their  Pythagorean  harmony,  15a 

[Spioe,  B.,  502^,  51 16] 

Spinet  [its  striking-place,  yyd] 

SpiUflbU,  organ  stop,  94a,  [94x2] 

[Spontini,  4976] 

[Staff  Notation,  426J'] 

[Stainer,  Dr.,  4976,  5056] 

Stapes^  see  Ear 

Stark,  Prof.,  241^ 

Stefan,  ♦37  7^ 

[Stein  of  Augsburg,  knew  nothing  of  a  uniform 
striking-place  for  piano  strings,  yyd,  S04d] 

Steinway  &  Sons  of  New  York,  their  piano, 
76a.  [strikes  at  vV  ftncl  i  the  length  of  string, 
ySd'.  the  9th  harmonic  obtained  by  Mr.  Hip- 
kins,  y6d'.    507(2,  5 1 1 6] 

Stereoscope  analyses  sensation  of  solidity,  63c 

Stirrup,  see  Ear 

[Stockhausen,  4956] 

Stokes,  Prof.,  ♦383d 

[Stone,  astronomer,  1730^ 

[Stone,  Dr.  W.  H.,  494(2.  his  restoration  of 
16  foot  C,  to  the  orchestra,  5526.  his  contra- 
fagotto,  553aT 

[Stoney,  Dr.  G.  J.,  on  characters  of  keys,  SSoc'] 

Stopped  pipes,  see  Organ  pipes  stopped 

Stops,  compound,  on  organ,  their  use,  206c. 
also  see  Organ  stops 

Stradivari,  1644- 1737,  his  violins,  866.  [re- 
sonance of  the  box  of  Dr.  Huggins*s  Stradi- 
vari violin  of  1708,  Syc^ 

Straight  lines  and  acute  angles  in  vibrational 
forms  how  produced,  34^2  35a 

[Strauss,  E.,  his  pitch,  555(2] 

Straw-fiddle,  a  wood  harmonioon,  71a 

[Stream  reeds,  554c] 

[Streatfield,  497a] 

[Streicher,  $02d] 

Striking  Beeds,  95c.    how  constructed,  96c2' 

Strings,  their  forms  of  vibration,  how  best 
studied,  456,  c.  number  of  nodes,  46c.  in- 
finite number  of  forms  of  vibration,  46(2. 
different  forms  excited  at  tiie  same  time,  47c. 
experiment  on,  with  a  flat  piano,  47c.  [on  a 
cottage  piano,  47(2.]  their  tones  best  adapted 
for  proving  the  ear's  analysis  of  compound 
tones  into  partials,  $2a,  motion  of,  when 
deflected  by  a  point,  53(2  to  54c.  excited  by 
striking,  746.  their  musical  tones,  74a.  how 
to  experiment  upon,  756.  of  pianoforte,  their 
qualities  of  tone,  7gc.  theoretical  intensity 
for  difference  of  hanmier  and  duration  of 
stroke  at  }  length  of  string,  79a,  h,  in  the 
upper  octaves  prime  predominant,  in  lower 
octaves  2nd  and  3rd  partial  louder  than  prime, 
80a.  effect  of  thickness  and  material,  8oa. 
motion  of  plucked,  mathematically  investiga- 
ted, 374(2  to  377a.  of  pianofortes,  vibrational 
forms  of,  mathematically  investigated,  380a. 
see  also  Pianoforte 

[Stroh,  *7Sd\  see  Preece,  124.  synthetical 
production  of  vowels,  542(2] 

Stroke,  for  exciting  string,  its  nature,  74(2. 
duration  of,  75c 

Subdominant  chord,  293.    [Duodene,  462a] 
[Snbminor  Pifth  5  :  7,  its  partials  examined, 

195c] 
Subminor  Fourteenth  2  :  7  much  better  than 
minor  Thirteenth,  196a 
Subminor  Tenth  3  :  7  much  better  than  minor 
Tenth,  196a 


Subminor  Seventh  4  :  7,  49i'.  often  more 
harmonious  than  minor  Sixth,  195a.  why 
not  used,  195a,  2136,  c.  its  partials  ex- 
amined, 195& 

[Subminor  Third  6  :  7,  its  partials  examined, 
195c] 

[Suction  chambers,  5556] 

Sumatra,  pentatonic  scale,  257 

Summational  combinational  tones  (Helm- 
holtz's),  153a.  only  heard  on  harmonium 
and  polyphonic  siren,  155c.  exemplified, 
156a.  are  very  inharmonic,  156&.  from 
polyphonic  siren  act  on  membranes,  157a. 
from  harmonium  act  partly  on  resonators, 
157c.  [reasons  for  doubting  the  two  last  con- 
clusions, 157(2] 

[Supermajor  Third  7  :  9,  its  partials  examined, 
I95fl 

[Superminor  Third  14  :  17,  its  partials  exa- 
mined, 195(2] 

[Supersecond  7  :  8,  its  partials  examined,  195c] 

Suspension  of  dissonant  note,  3546 

Sylvester,  Pope,  a.d.  314-335.  established  Bo- 
man  school  of  singing,  239a 

Sympathetic  oscillation  and  resonance,  its 
mechanics,  366.  of  piano  strings,  38(2.  of 
bodies  of  small  mass,  39c.  of  tuning-forks, 
39(2,  40a.  of  circular  membranes,  40C-41C. 
relation  between  its  strength  and  the  length 
of  time  required  for  the  tone  to  die  away, 
mathematic!ally  investigated,  405c 

Sympathetic  vibration,  the  only  analogue  to 
the  resolution  of  compound  into  simple  vibra- 
tions by  the  ear,  i2ga,  of  expansion  of 
auditory  nerves,  1426.  relation  of  amount  of, 
to  difference  of  pitch,  142c 


[Tadolini,  510(2] 

[Tagore,  Bajah  Sourindro  Mohun,  * 243(2. 
514c.    Indian  scale,  5i7(2] 

[Tambour,  Northern  and  Southern,  their  scales 
after  Prof.  Land,  517a] 

[T4r  of  Cashmere,  522a] 

Tartini,  1692- 1770,  Italian  violinist,  dis* 
covers  combinational  tones,  152c*.  his  theory 
of  consonance,  '232a.  estimated  all  combi- 
national tones  an  octave  too  high,  62a 

[Taskin,  Pascal,  Court  harpsichord  tuner,  5096] 

Taste,  difficulties  of  perceiving  analytically, 
636 

Taylor,  Sedley,  his  Sound  and  MusiCj  6c 

Tempered  fusion  of  just  intervals,  337(2 

Temperament,  relations  leading  to  it,  312&,  c* 
[App.  XX.  sect.  A,  see  contents  430(2] 

Terms  defined,  236,  c,  24a 

Terpander,  b.c.  700-650,  249a.  his  seven- 
stringed  cithara,  257<2.  his  scale  with  a 
tetrachord  and  Trichord,  26yd 

[Terei  suoni^  Tartini*s  name  for  combinational 
tones,  i$2d:\ 

Tetrachorids,  conjunct  and  disjunct,  255(1; 
i)  ancient  enharmonic  of  Ol3m:ipos,  2626; 
2)  older  chromatic,  262c ;  (3)  diatonic,  262c ; 
I4)  of  Didymus,  263a ;  (5)  Pythagorean,  2636; 
(6)  Phrygian,  263c ;  (7)  Lydian,  263(5 ;  (8)  un- 
used, 263(2;  (9)  soft  diatonic,  264a;  (10) 
Ptolemy *s  equal  diatonic,  2646;  (11)  enhar- 
monic, 2656.  [old  Greek,  51 2(2.  Greek  after 
Al  Farabi,  5I2(2] 

Tetrads,  or  four-part  chords,  when  consonant 
formed  by  taking  the  Octave  of  one  tone  of  a 
triad,  2226,  c,  223a.  major,  their  most  per- 
fect positions,  223c  j 

Digitized  by  V^OOQ  IC 


574 


INDEX. 


Thebes  in  Egypt,  flutes  fonnd  there,  27  id 

[TheUWne^'gaTi'toneB,  or  partial  tones,  240^ 

Third,  iii,  see  Major  Third  and  Minor  Third 

Thirds,  Pythagorean,  looked  on  as  normal 
Thirds  to  the  close  of  Middle  Ages,  190&. 
their  tempered  intonation  is  the  principal 
fault  of  tempered  intonation,  31  $6.  not  ad- 
mitted to  be  consonances  till  end  of  twelfth 
centary,  and  then  only  as  imperfect,  190a. 
[major  and  minor,  their  npper  partials  com- 
pared, i^od] 

Third  and  Fourth  triad,  3386 

Thirds  and  Sixths,  consonant  and  dissonant, 
enumerated  and  considered,  334a.  triad  of 
dissimilar,  3386 

Thirteenths  not  so  pleasant  as  Sixths,  1896. 
[partials  of  Sixths  and  Thirteenths  compared, 
1906] 

[Thomas's  orchestra,  Cincinnati,  U.S.,  51 16] 

Thompson,  Gen.  Perronet,  [his  life,  422dJ]  his 
enharmonic  organ,  422c.  its  effect,  423(2.  a 
soprano  voice  singing  to  it  and  a  blind  man 
playing  the  violin  with  it,  4236.  [his  mono- 
chord,  44id,    notes  on  his  organ,  473^2.] 

[Thompson,  Sir  W.,  his  electrical  squirting 
recorder,  539a] 

Thorough  Bass,  had  formerly  no  scientific 
foundation,  2c 

[Threequartertone  intervals,  525a] 

[Timbre,  its  proper  meaning,  why  not  used 
here  for  quality  of  tone,  24c  ] 

Timour,  282<i 

Toepler  andBoltzman,  their  experiments  on  the 
state  of  air  inside  flue-pipes,  *93a 

[Tomkins,  494^,  503a,  505c] 

Tonal  keyp  of  later  times,  270c 

Tonal  modes,  five  melodic,  2726.  Greek, 
formed  from  a  succession  of  7  Fifths,  288c. 
only  two  X)ossible  for  a  close  connection  of 
all  the  chords,  300a.  the  major  is  best,  300&. 
the  minor  is  second,  300^.  as  formed  from 
their  three  chords,  subdominant,  tonic,  and 
dominant,  293(2  to  294a.  their  chords  with 
double  intercalary  tones,  297c,  d,  with  single 
ditto,  2986,  c 

Tonal  relationship,  unconsoious  sense  of, 
370a 

Tonality  developed  in  modem  music,  5c.  the 
relation  of  all  the  tones  in  a  piece  to  the 
tonic  (F^tis),  240&.  Greeks  had  an  unde- 
veloped feeling  for,  242.  complete  in  major 
modes,  has  to  be  partly  abandoned  in  other 
modes,  2g6d.  [absent  in  non^harmonio  scales, 
526c] 

Tones,  (meaning  musical  sounds),  harmonic 
upper*  partial,  4d,  distinguished  by  force, 
pitch,  and  quality,  loc.  sharper  or  higher,  and 
flatter  or  lower,  116  [I  Ki'].  musical,  7c.  de- 
fined, 8a,  c,  236.  simple  and  compound,  de- 
fined, 236.  the  term '  tone  *  used  indifferently 
for  a  simple  or  compound  tone,  24a.  consti- 
tuent, of  a  chord,  24d\  composite,  $6d.  com- 
pound, 57a.  u]^per  partial,  a  general  consti- 
tuent of  all  musical  tones,  586,  c.  the  difiBculty 
of  hearing  them  does  not  depend  on  their 
weakness,  sSd,  circumstances  favourable  for 
distinguishing  musical  tones  from  different 
sources,  59c.  old  rules  of  composition  were 
designed  to  render  the  voice  parts  separable 
by  ear,  59(2.  without  upper  partials,  69c. 
with  inharmonic  upper  partials,  706.  of 
elastic  rods,  yod,  of  bowed  instruments,  80c. 
with  a  tolerably  loud  series  of  harmonic  par- 
tials to  Sixth  indusive,  are  most  harmonious, 
119a.     their   hollow    nasal,    poor,    cutting. 


rough  character,  whence  derived,  1 19a,  h. 
their  natural  relationship  as  a  basis  for 
scales,  2566.  related  to  dominant  ascending 
and  descending  scales,  2746,  c.  related  to 
subdominant  ascending  and  descending  scales, 
275a,  b.  of  voice,  energetic,  how  produced, 
115c.  [partial,  246.]  proper  not  generally 
determinable,  55(2,  but  determined  in  circular 
plates  and  stretched  membranes,  56a.  simply 
compounded,  tone  and  Octave  or  tone  and 
Twelfth,  305,  32J.  simple,  11 8(2.  simple,  how 
to  produce  by  means  of  resonance  jar,  54<2  to 
S$c,  combinational  {see  Combinational  tones) 

Tones  (meaning  musical  intervals),  printed 
with  a  capital  letter  for  distinction,  24c.  and 
Semitones  where  used,  $24d 

Tongue  (see  Beed) 

Tonic,  did  any  exist  in  homophonic  music  ? 
240&.  rules  for  finding  it  in  the  authentic 
scales  uncertain,  2436.  Greeks  used  any  note  as 
such,  268a.  chord  represents  compound  tone 
of  tonic,  296c.  chord,  development  of  feeling 
for  in  1 6th  and  17th  centuries,  2966.  feeling 
for  it,  weakly  developed  in  homophonic  music, 
243c 

Tonic  Sol-fa.  singers,  207(2.  teaches  to  sing 
by  the  characters  of  the  tones  in  the  scale, 
279(2.  system  of  shewing  relation  of  each 
note  to  the  tonic,  352(2.  society  of,  4236. 
[history  of,  423(2'  to  425c'  notes.  festiTiOs, 
42701 

[T6pfer,  509(2] 

[Translator,  his  additions  in  Appendix  XX., 
430  to  556J 

[Trautmann,  Moritz,  on  vowel  resonance,  109c', 
nod] 

Triads,  how  formed,  21 26,  e.  consonant  within 
the  compass  of  an  octave,  21 2(2.  see  Major 
and  Minor  triads,  of  two  just  major  Thirds, 
and  their  transformations,  338(2,  3396.    with 

^two  dissonances,  339c,  d.  how  they  limit 
the  tones  of  the  key,  340&,  c,  their  pos- 
sible confusions,  341c.  [cell  and  union,  of  a 
duodene,  and  condissonant  triads,  459a] 

Tremor,  sonorous,  distinct  from  motion  of  in- 
dividual particles  of  air,  8(2 

Trent,  council  of,  alters  music,  247a 

Trichordal  representation  of  harmonisabte 
modes,  ^ogd 

[Trichordals,  harmonic,  460a] 

[Trines,  major  and  minor,  pure  quintal,  major 
and  minor  quintal,  459a] 

[Tritonic  temperament,  true  and  false,  548a] 

Trombones,  lengthening,  looa.  [shape  ot 
looc'j 

Tropes  or  scales  of  the  best  Greek  period, 
essential,  having  hypate  as  tonic,  268e2 

[Trumpets,  their  shape,  looc.  with  slides, 
rare,  looc^ 

Tsay-yu,  said  to  have  introduced  the  hepte- 
tonic  scale  into  China,  258a 

Tso-kiu-ming  compares  the  five  Chinese  tonel 
to  the  five  Chinese  elements,  229c 

[Tunbridge,  496(2] 

Tune,  meniAi '^OemUthslimmung,  2$od 

Tunes,  popular,  constructed  from  the  three 
constituent  major  chords  of  the  scale,  292a 

[Tuning.  Sec.  G,  see  contents,  483(2.  its 
difficulties,  484(2.  examination  of  various  spe- 
cimens of,  4B4&  to  48^(2.  Translator's  prac- 
tical rule  for  tuning  in  equal  and  meantone 
intonation,  488(2  to  491c] 

Tuning-fork,  its  form  of  vibration,  20a,  d.  its 
tone,  79&.  has  high  inharmonic  proper  tones, 
70c.    large,  of  64  vib.  gat«,J^P93l*M^  '59«- 

Digitized  by^ 


LUXUU.IUVUJIU  |/«v^fcia   I 


INDEX. 


S7S 


[bow  to  treat  and  tune,  443a',  d\  tonometer, 
invented  by  Scbeibler,  443c.  best  method  of 
making,  443^2.  how  to  use,  444a.  the  Trane- 
lator*B,  446^^.]    &e€  also  Forks,  tuning 

Twelfth  gives  no  beats,  except  those  from 
partials  in  a  single  compound  tone,  iSy^f.  an 
absolute  consonance,  194&.  better  than  Fifth, 
1956.  repetition  in  it  presents  nothing  but 
what  has  already  been  heard,  2536 

Twelve  Semitones  introduced  into  China, 
2566 

Tympanum,  see  Drum 

Tyndall,  J.,  *on  sound,*  6c'.  translated  by 
Helmholtz  and  Wiedemann,  6€f.  [his  *  tone, 
clang,  clangtint,  overtone,'  24,  footnote.  '  re- 
sultant tones,*  1 53c.  finds  beats  do  not  in- 
crease the  range  of  power  of  sounds,  *i7gd, 
observations  on  gas  jets,  395a,  d] 


U 


U,  its  resonance  cavity,  io6a 

[TJeberzahn,  2$d'] 

[XJllmann,  504^'] 

XJlm,96d 

Unconscious  apprehension  of  regularity,  3676 

Undertones,  harmonic,  defined,  44d 

Undulatory  motion,  ga 

[Unequally  just  intonation,  465a] 

Unevenly  numbered  partials,  easier  to  observe 
than  the  evenly  numbered,  49c 

Unison  gives  no  beats  except  those  from  par- 
tials in  a  single  compound  tone,  iSyd,  greatly 
disturbs  adjacent  consonances,  i88a 

Upper  partials,  see  Tones,  upper  partials,  586 

[Upper  tooth,  2Sd'] 

Utficulus,  see  Ear 


[Vddiy  chief  Indian  note,  243c'.     or  *  ruler' 

note,  526c,  see  ans'd] 
[Variability   of    Seconds    and    Sevenths,   its 

effect  on  the  modes,  277c',  d"] 
Testibule  of  the  labyrinth,  i^sd 
fVianesi,  sood] 

Viadana  invents  solo  songs  with  airs,  2480 
Vibrating  forks,  phases  of,  as  compared  with 

those  of  the  exciting  current,  mathematically 

investigated,  4026 
Vibration   microscope,  dod.     curves    shewn, 

826 
Vibrational  form  or  form  of  vibration,    simple, 

2ia.    for  water-wheel  hammers  and  struck- 

up  balls,  2IC 
Vibrations,  86.    single,  as  reckoned  in  France, 

inconvenient    for    determining   pitch,    i6d'. 

form    of,    20a.    for  a  tuning-fork,    206,    c. 

pendular  or  simple,  23a 
Vibrator,  see  Beed 

[Vieth,  G.  U.  A.,  first  uses  the  term  •  combina- 
tional tones,*  *i53c] 
Villoteau,  believes  the  Eissar  to  be  penta- 

tonic,   ♦257^.    [his    thirds  of  Tones,   282^. 

origin  of  his  conception  of  thirds  of  Tones, 

5206,  55W] 
[Vince,  I35cfl 
Vi61a  box,  pitch  of  its  resonance  according  to 

Zamminer,  865,  [S6d'  note] 
Vidla  di  Oamba,  organ  stop,  93a,  [93d] 
Violin  string,  its  vibrational  form,  836.   crum- 
'pies  upon  it,  84c.    development  of  Octave, 


85c.  box,  the  pitch  of  its  resonance  accord- 
ing to  Savart,  reported  by  Zamminer,  866. 
[according  to  Translator's  observations,  876, 
c,  d,]  condition  for  regularity  of  vibration, 
Ssd.  why  old  ones  are  good,  Ssd,  bowing 
the  most  important  element,  86a.  sound- 
post,  its  function,  866.  [according  to  Dr. 
Huggins's  experiments  to  communicate  vi- 
brations from  belly  to  back,  *86c,  d.]  effect 
of  the  resonance  of  its  box  on  quality  of  its 
tones,  2ioa.  intonation  and  experiments, 
3246,  c.  Comn  and  Mercadier,  325^,  [486  to 
487].  strings,  their  motion  mathematically 
investigated,  384  to  387 

[Violinists,  their  intonation  as  determined  by 
Comu  and  Mercadier,  486c.  differi&nt  for 
harmony  and  melody,  487c] 

Vwlofi'bass  organ  stop,  93a,  [93J] 

Violoncello  organ  stop,  930,  [g^d] 

Violoncello  box,  pitch  of  its  resonance  accord- 
ing to  Savart  and  Zamminer,  866,  [86d'] 

Vis  viva,  insufficient  measure  of  the  strength 
of  tones,  1746,  d,  [75(f) 

Visoher,  his  *  Esthetics,'  26 

[Vocalists,  their  intonation,  difficulties  in 
actual  observation,  486a] 

Vocal  chords  or  ligaments,  98a.  their  rate  of 
vibration  not  iStered  materially  by  air- 
chambers,  their  watery  tissues,  and  variable 
thickness,  1006 

Vocal  expression,  natural  means  of,  370^ 

Voice,  well  suited  to  harmony,  206c.  effect  in 
certain  chords,  with  different  vowels,  2o6d. 
[compass  of  the  human,  5446] 

[Voice  Harmonium,  Colin  Brown's,  47od] 

[Voiceless  vowels,  6Sd] 

[Voicer,  the,  his  arts,  397iT 

[Voicing  of  reeds,  554^^ 

Vortical  surfaces,  3946 

Vowel  qualities  of  Tone,  103a.  their  charac- 
ter, 103a.  theory  fiTst  announced  by  Wheat- 
stone,  103d'.  produced  by  resonance  of  cavi- 
ties of  mouth,  104c.  trigram  of  du  Bois 
Beymond  the  elder,  1056.  resonance  recom- 
mended to  philologists  for  defining  vowels, 
io6c.  [difficulties  in  doing  so,  106c'.  differ* 
ences  of  opinion  of  Helmholtz,  Donders, 
Merkel,  Koenig,  io6d*,]  resonances  in  notes 
according  to  Helmholtz,  1 106.  their  recogni- 
tion by  resonators,  i  loc.  their  modifications, 
ii3« 

Vowels,  A,  1056.  O  U,  io6a,  [io6c,  d.]  O* 
and  A**,  106&.  the  above  have  only  a  single 
resonance,  io6c.  X,  E,  I,  double  resonance, 
107a,  6,  c.  [Graham  Bell  finds  double  reso- 
nance in  all  vowels,  lojd,]  A,  E,  I,  have 
their  resonances  too  high  for  forks,  1 1  la,  0, 
t!,  io8a,  5.  U,  how  Helmholtz  determined 
its  resonance,  iiO(|i.  its  degeneration  into 
Ou,  nob.  their  transitional  forms  due  to 
continuous  alterations  of  resonance  cavities, 
I  lie  to  ^^.  better  distinguished  when 
powerfttll^l^^mcnced,  ii^d,  their  quality 
of  tone,  115a.  distinguished  preponderantly 
by  depending  on  the  absolute  pitch  of  the 
partials  that  are  reinforced,  i  i8c.  their  effect 
on  harmony,  207a  [c,  d],  practical  direc- 
tions for  their  synthesis,  398a  to  400c.  [their 
new  analysis  by  means  of  the  phonograph  by 
Messrs.  Jenkin  and  Ewing,  538  to  542.  Oh 
analysed,  539  to  541.  Oo,  Aw,  Ah,  analysed, 
541.  their  synthetical  production  by  Messrs. 
Preece  and  Stroh,  542^].  echoed  from  piano, 
61C.  vowels  only,  without  consonants,  heard 
from  speakers  at  a  disti^pce,  68c 

Digitized  by^OOQlC 


576 


INDEX. 


W 


Wagner,  B.,  his  treatment  of  the  chord  of  two 
major  Thirds,  3396.  [thinks  in  equal  tem- 
perament, 339<2'.    his  festival,  502c] 

[Walcker  of  Ludwigsburg,  had  little  experience 
of  striking  reeds,  96c] 

Waldeyer  finds  4500  outer  arch  fibres  of  Corti 
in  the  cochlea  [giving  i  for  each  2  cents],  1476 

Waller,  B.,  1686,  reduces  all  colours  to  three 
fundamental,  64a 

[Walker,  J.  W.  &  Sons,  organ-builders,  never 
used  free  reeds,  96c.  5066^  506c,  507a.  in 
July  1852  put  the  Exeter  Hall  organ  in 
equal  temperament,  549a'] 

Water-wheel  hammer,  its  periodic  motion,  190, 
21C 

Waves  of  water,  96,  d.  generated  by  a  regular 
series  of  drops,  loa.  of  rope,  chain,  india- 
rubber  tubing,  brass  wire  spiral,  9c.  their 
composition,  256,  c,  26a-{2.  of  water  or  air, 
algebraical  addition,  27^,  28c.  periodic,  re 
suiting  from  composition  of  simple  tones, 
y>b  to  32^.  phases  of,  caused  by  resonance, 
40(x2,  mathematically  investigated,  401a, 
of  the  sea,  effect  of  their  motion  on  speota* 
tors,  251a 

[Weber,  Frank  Anton  von,  497a,  509&] 

[Weber,  0.  M.  von,  498c] 

Weber,  Dr.  Fr.  E.,  on  function  of  the  aqtUB- 
ductus  vesHb&H,  136a 

Weber,  W.,  390* 

Weitzmann,  *26gc,  d,  title 

Werckmeister  (b.  1645),  advocated  equal  tem- 
perament in  1 69 1,  321  c,  548c 

Wertheim,  3736 

Westphal,  •265^',  ♦268c,  d 

Wetness,  sensation  of,  compounded  of  un- 
resisting gliding  and  cold,  63c 

Wheatstone,  Sir  Charles,  first  announces  a 
vowel  theory,  ♦103d.  [repeats  Willis's  ex- 
periments on  vowel  reproduction,  117^] 

Whispered  vowels,  pitch  of,  io8c.  Gzermak 
and  Merkel  on,  108^' 

[White,  J.  Paul,  his  Harmon,  22&2,  329^'. 
described,  481 6.  his  methods  of  tuning  it, 
492c] 

Wiedemann,  G.,  ^dc' 


[Wilkies,  the,  5496I 

[Willis,  organ-builder,  never  uses  free  re«4i, 

96c,  5065,  506c,  507a] 
Willis,  Prof.  B.,  *io2d\    his  reproductioii  d 

vowels  by  extensible  reed  pipes,  11 76.   iUt 

table  of  vowel  resonance,  117c.    his  ezpiri- 

ments  with  toothed  wheels  and  springs,  1 18a. 

his  vowel  theory,  1186 

Winterfeld,   von,  *24Sd,  ♦272^,  287^',  *z^ 
[Withers,    violin-maker,    assists    in    findbig 

resonance  of  violin,  876,  c] 
[W6lfel,  5096] 

[Woneggar's  abstract  of  Glarean,  196(2'] 
Wooden  instruments  are  mobile  in  tone,  67^. 

pipes  have  softer  tone  than  metal  pipes,  94c 

reeds,  gSb 

Wood  harmonicon,  71a 
[Woolhouse's  cycle  of,  19,  436(2] 


[Ynignez,  Don,  organist  of  Seville,  496a] 

Young,  T.,  77d' 

Young,  Thomas,  1773-1829,^8  law  that  ezdt- 
ing  a  string  at  a  node  destroys  the  harmoniei 
corresponcUng  to  that  node,  526.  its  proof, 
52c.  536  [S^Z^j  5466.]  his  analysis  of  eokwr 
into  3  primaries,  14BC,  149c.  his  theory  U 
differential  tones  generated  by  beats,  i66i 


Z 


Zamminer,  *62d,  on  pitch  of  resonance  of  violia, 
violoncello,  and  vi61a  boxes,  866  [*S6d'  note.] 
length  of  horn,  99c.  [error  in  reporting, 
lood  note.]    323^2,  3906,  394a 

[Zaizal,  lutist  introduces  ThreequartertflBO, 
264^',  2Sid,  d,  his  two  new  intervals  of  355 
and  853  cents,  28i(2,  6^,  5256] 

Zarlino  assumes  the  tenor  voice  part  to  deter- 
mine the  key,  245(i,  312a,  326a,  t35i(i'.  hii 
temperament,  546^] 

Zillerthal,  in  Tyrol,  scale  of  its  wood  haniMi- 
icon,  270(2 

Zither,  746 

Z6nak  dmticula'iat  i^gd 


PRINTED    BT 

•POTnflW(X)DS   AKD    CO.,    K£W-STREET  bQUARB 

LOMDOS 

Digitized  by 


Google 


Digitized  by 


Google 


Digitized  by 


Google 


Digitized  by 


Google       _ 


:,:    V::^/ 


v 


^  -■>•'**;• 


7y, 


-i^ 


% 


'  '-rv^ 


x^^ 


•  C\  .1  ^•-  •>•,  ki  . "«  ^'-  -'^  O 


'il 


/;,■.'/ 


.<^ 

t"*- 

.•^>- 

c 

y 

r 

fy/ 

--S'    ^■■:^^'! 


% 

^-f' 


'^ 


.4' 


\    -'vV 


r'  h- 


■  V  ./ 


s>t. 


Cu 


f'^f    /:.::    NVl 


1^  1 


oi 


-v. 


^O  -<-,:.l>jl>«"','^  O,        Digitized  by 

/v^'i'     ^;f-    \':i^ 


s^ 


■1 


\it)OQle