Skip to main content

Full text of "The physico-chemical mechanism of nerve activity"

See other formats


ANNALS  OF  THE  NEW  YORK  ACADEMY  OF  SCIENCES 
Volume  XLVII.  Art.  4.     Pages  375-602 


Editor 

Roy  Waldo  Miner 

Consulting  Editor 

David  Nachmansohm 

Associate  Editor 
Michael  Demarest 


THE  PHYSICO-CHEMICAL  MECHANISM 
OF  NERVE  ACTIVITY 


By 


David  Nachmansohn,  Charles  M.  Berry,  Oscar  Bodansky,  Frank 

Brink,  Jr.,  Detlev  W.  Bronk,  M.  Vertner  Brown,  C.  W.  Coaxes, 

R.  T.  Cox,  J.  C.  Eccles,  Alfred  Fessard,  J.  F.  Fulton,  R.  AV. 

Gerard,  Alfred  GilxMAN,  D.  E.  Green,  Joseph  C.  Hinsey, 

Rudolf  Hober,  Martin  G.  Larrabee,  and 

Tracy  J.  Putnam 


Q 

11 

.N5 


new  YORK 

Published  by  the  Academy 

DFrrMBFR  15,  1946 


Annals  of  The  New  York  Academy  of  Sciences 

Volume  XLVII,  Art.  4.     Pages  375-602 

December  15,  1946 

THE  PHYSICO-CHEMICAL  MECHANISM 
OF  NERVE  ACTIVITY* 

By 

David  Nachmansohn,  Charles  M.  Berry,  Oscar  Bodansky,  Frank 

Brink,  Jr.,  Detlev  W.  Bronk,  M.  Vertner  Brown,  C.  W.  Coates, 

R.  T.  Cox,  J.  C.  Eccles,  Alfred  Fessard,  J.  F.  Fulton,  R.  W. 

Gerard,  Alfred  Oilman,  D.  E.  Green,  Joseph  C.  Hinsey, 

Rudolf  Hober,  Martin  G.  Larrabee,  and 

Tracy  J.  Putnam 


CONTENTS 

PAGE 

Introduction  to  the  Conference  on  Nerve  Activity.     By  Tracy  J.  Putnam  377 

The  Membrane  Theory.     By  Rudolf  Hober 381 

Chemical  Mechanism  of  Nerve  Activity.     By  David  Nachmansohn 395 

An  Electrical  Hypothesis  of  Synaptic  and  Neuro-Muscular  Tr.-vnsmis- 

siON.     By  J.  C.  Eccles 429 

Chemical  Excitation  of  Nkrve.     By  Frank  Brink,  Jr.,  Detlev  W.  Bronk, 

AND  Martin  G.  L.\rrabee 457 

Electrical  Characteristics  of  Electric  Tissue.     By  R.  T.  Cox,  C.  W. 

Coates,  and  M.  Vertner  Brown 487 

Some  Basic  Aspects  of  the  .Activity  of  Electric  Plates.     By  .4.. Fessard.  .   501 

Physiological  Function  from  the  Standpoint  of  Enzyme  Chemistry.     By 

D.  E.  Green '. 515 

Cholinesterase.     By  Oscar  Bodansky 521 

The  Effects  of  Drugs  on  Nerve  Activity.     By  Alfred  Gilman 519 

The  Recovery  of  Diameter  and  Impulse  Conduction  in  Regenerating 

Nerve  Fibers.     By  Charles  M.  Berry  .\nd  Joseph  C.  Hinsey 553 

Nerve  Metabolism  and  Function.     By  R.  W.  Gerard 575 

Conclusion.     By  J.  F.  Fulton 601 

*  This  .series  of  papers  is  tlie  result  of  a  Conference  on  The  Physico -Chemical  Mechanism  of 
Nerve  Activity  held  by  the  Section  of  Biologv  of  The  New  York  Academy  of  Sciences,  February  8 
and  9,  1946. 

Publication   made  possible  through    a   grant   from  the  Conference  EuWie&tiQjis   Revolving  Fund. 

(375)  Xv^3^^i47  N 


fi 


library)  5i 


Copyright  1946 

BY 

The  New  York  Academy  of  Sciences 


INTRODUCTION  TO  THE  CONFERENCE 
ON  NERVE  ACTIVITY 

By  Tracy  J.  Putnam 
Columbia  University,  New  York,  N.  Y. 

It  is  my  pleasant  duty  to  open  a  conference  which  is,  in  several 
respects,  historic. 

In  the  first  place,  this  is,  as  far  as  I  am  aware,  the  first  truly  inter- 
national conference  on  a  purely  physiologic  or,  in  the  broad  sense, 
medical  subject,  since  the  beginning  of  the  war.  I  see,  in  the  audience, 
five  continents  represented,  and  I  can  reproach  the  program  commit- 
tee and  myself  only  for  failing  to  arrange  for  a  delegate  from  Africa. 
It  is  my  special  privilege  to  welcome  Dr.  Eccles  from  Australia,  Dr. 
Feng  from  China,  Drs.  Fessard,  Couteaux,  and  Bugnard  from  France, 
and  Dr.  Bremer  from  Belgium,  who  have  come  so  far  for  the  special 
purpose  of  taking  part  in  this  symposium. 

The  fact  that  we  are  all  met  here  from  various  corners  of  the  earth 
to  discuss  problems  of  pure  science  with  a  humanitarian  purpose  sym- 
bolizes, it  seems  to  me,  the  hope  of  this  troubled  world,  the  hope  that 
civilization  is  beginning  to  recover  from  a  desperate  and  destructive 
illness,  which  barely  missed  being  fatal.  A  relapse  might  well  be  final. 
But  here  we  are,  ready,  willing,  and  able  to  talk  over  some  questions 
which  are  of  great  importance,  but  definitel}^  non-political.  It  is  to  be 
expected  that  differences  of  opinion  will  arise,  and  facts  will  be  hotly 
debated.  I  feel  certain,  nevertheless,  that  the  members  of  this  confer- 
ence will  be  able  to  set  an  example  for  future  international  discussions, 
in  agreeing  on  the  criteria  of  truth  and  the  means  of  arriving  at  an 
agreement  on  facts,  with  a  broad  tolerance  towards  possible  means  of 
interpretation.  Let  us  hope  that  the  United  Nations  Organization  will 
take  notice  and  be  willing  to  learn. 

This  meeting  opens  a  wholly  new  vista,  in  another  sense,  also.  At 
the  time  when  the  German  Army  crossed  the  Polish  border,  in  1939, 
there  seemed  to  be  little  hope  of  bridging  the  gap  between  the  point  of 
view  that  transmission  of  the  nervous  impulse  was  a  purely  electrical 
phenomenon,  and  on  the  other  hand,  the  conception  that  the  production 
of  a  specific  chemical  substance  was  the  essential  fact.  The  atmosphere 
of  the  war  was  not  in  the  least  conducive  to  placid  scientific 

(377)  '  "^^ 


378  ANNALS  NEW  YORK  ACADEMY  OF  SCIENCES 

but  here,  six  years  have  passed,  and  behold!  the  whole  subject  has  be- 
come suddenly  clearer.  Our  new  insight  into  the  problem  has  not 
come  easily,  and  I  must  pay  special  tribute  to  our  colleagues  in  Europe 
and  in  China,  who  have  had  the  courage  to  carry  on  their  investigations 
in  the  face  of  enormous  discouragements  and  practical  difficulties,  and 
sometimes  even  in  secrecy  and  at  the  peril  of  their  lives. 

It  is  amazing  that  so  much  has  been  accomplished  under  such  ad- 
verse circumstances.  The  details  of  the  physiology  of  the  individual 
elements  of  the  nervous  system  have,  in  the  past,  seemed  most  obscure, 
and  we  have  had  to  infer  the  outlines  of  the  metabolic  processes  which 
occurred  there  from  data  gathered  from  other  tissues.  But  it  now 
seems  safe  to  say  that  our  picture  of  the  metabolism  and  the  mecha- 
nism of  action  of  neurones  is  more  complete  than  our  knowledge  of 
any  other  tissue,  and  the  methods  of  study  which  were  originally  con- 
fined to  neurophysiology  are  being  extended  to  other  physiologic 
problems. 

It  is  particularly  gratifying  to  me  that  a  clinical  neurologist  should 
be  permitted  to  open  this  meeting.  Clinical  neurology  used  to  be  con- 
sidered a  purely  diagnostic  specialty,  a  hopeless  field  of  medicine, 
which  consisted  in  little  more  than  a  meditation  on  disease.  We  are 
beginning  now  to  be  able  to  do  a  little  more  about  the  disorders  of  the 
nervous  system,  but  we  can  make  progress  only  as  we  possess  insight. 
The  physiologic  methods  of  study  which  have  been  devised  and  applied 
by  the  distinguished  scientists  I  see  before  me,  and  the  facts  they  have 
elicited  are,  I  am  sure,  the  surest  guide  we  possess  to  advances  in 
therapeutic  methods.  This  is  a  new  chapter,  not  only  in  neurophysi- 
ology, but  in  pharmacology,  clinical  medicine,  and,  perhaps,  even  for 
the  dark  territories  of  psychiatry. 

We  are  grateful  to  The  New  York  Academy  of  Sciences,  and  especially 
to  the  executive  secretary,  Mrs.  Miner,  for  having  organized  so  eflfi- 
ciently  and  made  possible  this  symposium.  We  were  fortunate  in  hav- 
ing the  support  of  the  Rockefeller  Foundation,  and  we  express  our 
gratitude  to  Dr.  Lambert  for  his  advice  and  active  cooperation. 

I  should  also  like  to  thank  very  warmly  Dr.  Raymond  Zwemer  who, 
through  his  association  with  the  State  Department,  helped  us  to  over- 
come many  difficulties. 

A  few  words  about  the  program.  The  purpose  of  the  symposium  is 
not  to  present  recent  data  alone,  but  to  give  an  opportunity  for  dis- 
cussing some  of  the  fundamental  aspects  and  problems.  There  is  so 
much  to  say  that  we  have  filled  the  program  perhaps  unduly  full,  and 
still  have  been  unable  to  find  time  for  many  investigators  whom  we 


PUTNAM:  INTRODUCTION  379 

should  all  enjoy  hearing.  We  hope,  however,  there  will  be  enough 
time  left  for  discussion,  if  we  abide  closely  by  our  schedule.  The 
chemical  aspect  of  the  subject  has  been  as  much  stressed  as  the  physical. 
It  seems  that  we  shall  all  have  to  get  accustomed  to  terms  like  enzymes 
and  coenzymes,  as  well  as  to  positive  and  negative  phases,  and  Weden- 
sky  inhibition.  At  the  end  of  the  symposium,  Dr.  Gerard  will  try  to 
integrate  the  different  aspects  which  will  be  presented  and  discussed. 


THE  MEMBRANE  THEORY 

By  Rudolf  Hober 
University  oj  Pennsylvania,  Philadelphia,  Pennsylvania 

The  classical  objects  of  the  study  of  bioelectric  phenomena  are  mus- 
cle and  nerve.  Resting,  injury,  and  action  potentials  and  currents  are 
studied  with  both  of  them.  On  the  basis  of  Wilhelm  Ostwald's  investi- 
gations upon  the  electric  properties  of  artificial  inorganic  precipitation 
membranes  (1890),  the  physiological  membrane  potentials  have  been 
looked  upon  as  being  special  forms  of  Nernst  concentration  potentials; 
in  other  words,  potentials  arising  when  solutions  of  different  electrolytes 
are  separated  by  a  membrane  characterized  by  a  more  or  less  selective 
ion  permeability.  As  it  was  from  the  beginning  of  the  electrophysio- 
logical era,  both  nerve  and  muscle  have  been  used  for  solving  the  basic 
problems,  and  information  gained  from  one  type  is  valuable  for  both. 
Therefore,  although  our  object  is  primarily  a  discussion  of  physico- 
chemical  mechanisms  of  nerve  activity,  muscle  potentials  will  also  be 
treated. 

The  Membrane  Theory  was  established,  in  1902,  by  Bernstein,  when 
he  ascribed  the  EMF  of  the  locally  injured  muscle  fiber  to  selective 
permeability  to  potassium  ions  present  inside  the  fiber  in  a  considerably 
greater  concentration  than  outside.  He  conceived  of  the  action  poten- 
tial wave  as  a  self-propagating  depolarization  by  breakdown  of  this 
selective  permeability.  It  was  early  assumed  that  this  alteration  is 
accompanied  by  chemical  reactions. 

THE  ION  PERMEABILITY  OF  THE  RESTING  MUSCLE 

AND  NERVE 

In  immediate  connection  with  Bernstein's  theory,  in  order  to  become 
acquainted  with  the  general  properties  of  the  selective  ion  permeable 
membranes,  it  seemed  to  me  that  one  of  the  main  tasks  was  to  study 
the  effect  of  local  application  of  the  neutral  inorganic  salts  upon  the 
resting  potential  of  muscle  (Hober^). 

A.     The  Inorganic  Cations 

At  first,  the  alkali  cations  only  were  varied,  and  potassium  was 
found  to  produce  the  strongest  negative  pole,  similar  to  the  effect  of 

(381) 


382  ANNALS  NEW  YORK  ACADEMY  OF  SCIENCES 

cutting  the  muscle,  but  often  as  a  reversible  process  and  obviously  in- 
dicative of  the  greatest  permeating  power.  The  other  alkali  cations 
appeared  to  be  less  effective,  in  this  order:  potassium,  rubidium, 
sodium,  lithium;  potassium  and  rubidium  producing  negativity,  as 
compared  to  sodium,  while  lithium  produces  positivity.  This  was 
interpreted  as  being  due  to  swelling  or  shrinking  of  hydrophilic  colloids, 
which  were  assumed  to  be  the  chief  constituents  of  the  plasma  mem- 
brane. Later,  the  same  series  was  met  by  Michaelis  with  non-colloidal, 
rigid,  dried  collodion  membranes.  His  findings  seemed  to  fit  in  best 
with  the  concept  of  an  ionic  sieve.  For,  taking  into  consideration  the 
shells  of  water  dipoles  around  the  ions,  the  effective  ionic  volume  ap- 
pears to  be  smallest  with  potassium,  largest  with  lithium.  But,  as 
will  be  seen  later,  the  differences  are  better  correlated  with  adsorption, 
which,  according  to  Gouy,  Frumkin,  and  others,  increases  with  decreas- 
ing hydration  of  the  ions,  potassium  being  most,  lithium  least,  adsorbed. 
The  membrane  theory  postulates  that,  as  in  the  case  of  the  suffi- 
ciently dried  collodion  membrane,  which  is  permeable  only  to  potassium 
ion,  strength  and  direction  of  the  injury  potential  are  dependent  upon 
the  ratio  of  potassium  inside  to  potassium  outside.  In  other  words, 
the  surface  of  muscle  or  nerve  behaves  as  a  potassium-electrode, 
potassium  inside  being  constant  and  about  20  to  40  times  greater  than 
potassium  outside.  Therefore,  by  raising  potassium  outside,  the  EMF 
of  an  injured  (cut)  muscle  should  be  decreased  to  zero,  if  potassium 
outside  is  equal  to  potassium  inside,  and  its  direction  should  be  re- 
versed, if  potassium  outside  is  greater  than  potassium  inside.  My  own 
early  experiments  (1905)  failed  to  show  the  reversal,  because,  in  con- 
trast to  the  rigid,  dried  collodion  membrane,  the  ion  selectivity  of  the 
plasma  membrane  is  lost,  due  to  its  colloidal  behavior:  in  other  words, 
due  to  the  swelling  and  disintegrating,  even  to  the  cytolyzing  effect  of 
higher  potassium,  especially  after  some  lapse  of  time.  However,  the 
postulate  of  a  reversal  complies  with  recent  observations  of  Hodgkin 
and  Huxley,'  and  of  Curtis  and  Cole,"  in  a  particularly  striking  way. 
These  authors,  leaning  upon  Osterhout's*  studies  on  the  "impaled" 
giant  plant  cells  ( Valonia) ,  pushed  a  microelectrode  into  the  axoplasma 
of  the  giant  nerve  fiber  of  the  squid,  along  its  axis,  so  that  its  tip  was 
placed  just  opposite  to  the  outside  electrode,  and  they  thus  measured 
the  membrane  potential  directly  across  the  wall.  The  potential  was 
found,  in  the  case  of  the  squid  nerve,  to  be,  on  an  average,  50  mV. 
Then,  upon  raising  potassium  outside  to  about  18  times  normal,  the 
resting  potential  was  decreased  to  zero,  and  upon  raising  it  about  40 
times  normal,  a  reversal  of  15  mV  was  observed.     The  corresponding 


HOBER:  THE  MEMBRANE  THEORY  383 

procedure  has  been  applied  to  single  frog  muscle  fibers  by  Gerard, 
Carlson,  and  Graham.^ 

B.     The  Inorganic  Anions 

The  colloidal  behavior  of  the  physiological  membranes,  further,  is 
brought  into  evidence  by  the  effect  of  the  inorganic  anions.  In  gen- 
eral, with  regard  to  their  role  in  physiology,  the  anions  are  less  powerful 
than  the  cations.  This  is  due  to  the  prevalent  negative  charge  of  the 
colloidal  aggregates,  which  repel  the  anions.  Locally  applied  to  nerve 
or  muscle,  the  resting  potential  reveals  the  following  anion  series:  thio- 
cyanide,  iodide,  bromide  and  chloride,  sulfate,  with  thiocyanide  ion 
producing  a  positive  pole.  In  other  words,  a  reversed  injury  potential 
occurs  as  a  consequence  of  the  anion  adsorption  on  the  membrane, 
which  is  greatest  with  thiocyanide,  smallest  with  sulfate.  This  re- 
versal is  an  important  point  to  be  kept  in  mind  for  later  discussion. 
However,  after  some  lapse  of  time  or  after  applying  the  salts  in  stronger 
solution,  the  anion  series  is  reversed,  thiocyanide  and  iodide  forming 
a  negative  pole,  thus  resembling  the  effect  of  potassium  ion.  This, 
again,  is  significant,  as  due  to  a  loosening,  softening,  and  subsequent 
disintegrating  action  upon  the  hydrophilic  colloidal  membrane.^'  ^ 

C.     Organic  Anions 

These  effects  are  related  to  those  of  a  large  group  of  organic  anions: 
for  instance,  those  of  higher  fatty  acids  starting  with  the  8-carbon  atom 
chain,  i.e.,  caprylic  acid.  These  ions  have  a  nonpolar-polar  structure, 
the  nonpolar,  or  organophilic  and  hydrophobic,  part  of  the  anion, 
mainly  due  to  the  adsorption  affinities  of  the  alkyl  radicals,  attaching  to 
the  organic  material,  e.g.,  to  the  particles  of  a  Langmuir  surface  film 
of  protein;  the  other  polar  or  hydrophilic  part,  due  to  the  carboxyl 
radicals  with  their  cloud  of  water  dipoles,  anchoring  in  the  water. 
It  appears  that  a  pull  towards  the  water  can  be  exhibited  upon  the 
organic  material.  The  pull  is  stronger  or  weaker,  corresponding  to  the 
relative  hydroaffinity,  so  that  colloidal  particles  composed  of  a  variety 
of  molecules,  hke  hemoglobin  (which  means,  hem  plus  globin),  visual 
purple,  or  chloroplastin,  can  be  torn  to  pieces,  a  process  termed  de- 
naturation,  solubilization,  or  detergency.  On  the  other  hand,  coiled 
peptide  chains  can  be  uncoiled,  and  organic  architectures  of  great  com- 
plexity, such  as  a  plasma  membrane,  can  be  loosened  by  the  adsorptive 
pull,  which  has  the  effect  of  abolishing  reversibly  the  selective  ion 
permeability  of  the  membrane  and  starting  irreversible  cytolysis.^ 
For  example,  sodium  caprylate  is  applied  locally  to  a  muscle.     The  re- 


384  ANNALS  NEW  YORK  ACADEMY  OF  SCIENCES 

suit  of  a  very  weak  solution  is  simply  a  reversed  resting  potential,  due 
to  the  anion  attachment  to  the  pores  of  the  membrane;  the  effect  of  a 
stronger  solution  is  a  regular  resting  potential,  due  to  reversible 
loosening;  while  the  result  of  a  still  stronger  solution  is  an  irreversible 
disintegration,  i.e.,  an  injury  potential. 

D.     Inorganic  Plurivalent  Cations 

The  occurrence  of  the  well-known  cation  antagonism  is  another  in- 
dication of  the  prevalence  of  anionic  colloids  in  cell  structure.  An  im- 
balance between  the  monovalent  and  the  plurivalent  cations  shows  up 
in  numerous  observations  upon  animal  and  plant  cells,  among  others 
by  alteration  of  their  electrical  properties,  as,  for  example,  ohmic 
resistance,  or  conductance,  or  excitability.  Preponderance  of  mono- 
valent cations  (sodium,  potassium)  is  bound  up  in  muscle  and  nerve 
with  loss  of  normal  selective  cation  permeability,  due  to  increased  hy- 
dration. However,  this  is  compensated  for  by  the  consolidating  effect 
of  plurivalent  cations,  like  calcium,  strontium,  barium,  cobalt,  manga- 
nese, nickel  (Hober^). 

THE  CHANGE  OF  ION  PERMEABILITY  BY  DC 

Let  us  turn  now  to  the  old  and  complex  phenomenon  of  electrotonus. 
It  comprises  a  multitude  of  alterations  of  cell  responses,  effected  by 
direct  current  and  having  their  origin  in  changes  of  membrane  polariza- 
tion. Especially  well-known  are  the  changes  of  excitability  of  nerve 
and  muscle.  Excitability  is  diminished  at  the  anode  and  increased 
at  the  cathode,  except  that,  beyond  a  certain  current  strength,  the  in- 
crease turns  to  a  decrease,  the  so-called  cathodic  depression.  Anelcc- 
trotonus  and  catelectrotonus  are  tied  up  with  changes  of  resistance. 
By  placing  one  electrode  on  an  intact  spot  of  the  excised  nerve,  the 
other  on  a  crushed  end,  the  resistance  is  raised  at  the  anode,  dimin- 
ished at  the  cathode.  Consequently,  while  sending  alternating  current 
through  the  preparation  instead  of  direct  current,  a  rectifier  effect  ap- 
pears. These  and  other  observations  can  be  explained,  partly  on  the 
basis  of  ion  distribution  between  the  inside  and  the  outside  of  the 
membranes,  partly  by  taking  again  into  account  the  colloidal  prop- 
erties of  the  wall  of  the  natural  membranes.  As  to  the  first  point,  ac- 
cording to  Bear  and  Schmitt,^  Cowan,^"  Fenn  and  others,"  and  Webb 
and  Young,^  the  axoplasm  of  the  giant  nerve  fiber  of  the  squid,  for 
example,  contains  4  to  5  times  more  inorganic  cations  (mainly  potas- 
sium) than  anions,  and  18  times  more  potassium  than  the  blood.     Be- 


HOBER:  THE  MEMBRANE  THEORY  385 

sides  chloride,  there  are  in  the  axoplasm  small  concentrations  of  phos- 
phate, sulfate,  and  lactate,  but  rather  large  amounts  of  organic  anions  of 
low  mobility,  possibly  the  anions  of  amino  acids.  These  conditions  are 
roughly  reproduced  in  model  experiments  of  Labes^^  and  Ebbecke.^^  A 
membrane  core-conductor  is  formed  by  a  collodion  tube,  with  pores  wide 
enough  to  allow  cations  and  anions  to  pass  the  wall.  The  tube  is  filled 
with  a  solution  of  potassium  phosphate  and  is  packed  in  gauze  which  has 
been  wetted  with  a  solution  of  sodium  chloride.  One  electrode  is  placed 
inside,  another  outside.  If  direct  current  passes  the  membrane,  a  smaller 
resistance  is  encountered,  when  the  current  goes  from  within  outwards, 
than  when  it  goes  in  the  opposite  direction.  The  reason  is  that,  with  the 
outgoing  current,  the  faster  potassium  inside  and  chloride  outside  are 
swept  into  the  membrane  and  travel,  there,  with  greater  velocity  than  so- 
dium outside  and  phosphate  inside,  being  driven  by  an  ingoing 
current.  If,  instead  of  collodium,  hydrophilic  and  negatively  charged 
colloids  are  the  membrane  constituents,  as  they  actually  are  under  most 
physiological  conditions,  then  additional  swelling  and  increasing  dis- 
persion occur  at  the  cathode,  as  well  as  shrinking  at  the  anode.  Con- 
sequently, the  polarizability  of  the  membrane  falls  at  the  cathode  and 
rises  at  the  anode.  Swelling  causes  in  natural  objects,  such  as  muscle 
and  nerve,  greater  excitability  at  the  cathode,  but  as  the  current 
strength  rises  more  and  more,  the  higher  excitability  turns  over  to 
inexcitability,  or,  in  other  words,  to  cathodic  depression.  More  spe- 
cifically, according  to  Blinks,^*  the  membrane  polarization  of  a  giant 
cell  of  the  fresh  water  alga  Nitella,  comparable  to  nerve  or  muscle  with 
its  thread-like  shape,  drops  down  to  zero,  if  exposed  to  the  swelling  effect 
of  potassium  chloride  in  sufficiently  high  concentration,  and  the  cell 
does  not  respond  any  more  to  otherwise  effective  stimuli.  How- 
ever, by  applying,  locally,  an  anode  of  rising  strength  to  the  depolarized 
Nitella  cell,  above  a  certain  threshold  value,  the  polarizability  is  re- 
stored, and  a  normal  action  potential  can  be  elicited  upon  stimulation. 
Alternatively,  with  respect  to  cation  antagonism,  after  excitability  of 
a  nerve  has  been  suppressed  by  calcium,  this  stiffening  effect  is  can- 
celled by  the  softening  influence  of  a  cathode,  as  shown  by  Woronzow," 
and  more  recently  by  Guttman  and  Cole.^^ 

We  turn,  now,  to  the  discussion  of  natural  changes  of  ion  permeability 
during  action.  It  has  been  accepted,  for  more  than  40  years,  that 
depolarization,  which  is  brought  about  by  injury,  compares  essentially 
to  depolarization  accompanying  excitation,  as  indicated  by  the  "nega- 
tive variation"  of  du  Bois-Reymond.  The  negativity  wave,  therefore, 
is  interpreted  as  a  "breakdown  of  the  membrane,"  by  which  the  selec- 


386  ANNALS  NEW  YORK  ACADEMY  OF  SCIENCES 

tive  cation  permeability  is  abolished.  Correspondingly,  it  has  been 
assumed  that,  during  excitation,  the  potential  fall  is  as  great  as  is  the 
resting  potential,  measured  at  best  with  the  impaled  nerve  or  muscle. 
But  this  is  not  true.  Impedance  measurements  have  shown  the  resist- 
ance to  persist,  to  some  extent,  during  excitation  (Curtis  and  Cole) .  In 
other  words,  the  resting  potential  could  be  expected  to  be  larger  than 
the  action  potential.  However,  the  contrary  is  true.  Hodgkin  and 
Huxley,-  and  Curtis  and  Cole,"  inserting  a  microelectrode  into  the  axon, 
detected  the  potential  change,  during  activity,  to  be  even  larger  than 
that  due  to  injury.  For  example,  in  the  experiments  of  Curtis  and 
Cole,  the  resting  potential  average  is  51  mV,  the  action  potential 
108  mV. 

Before  discussing  this  interesting  situation,  attention  will  be  turned 
briefly  to  a  special  problem.  The  word,  breakdown,  suggests  leakage, 
and  for  this  reason,  activity  could  be  expected  to  be  accompanied  by 
leakage,  especially  from  the  large  surplus  of  well-diffusible  potassium 
.normally  retained  in  the  axoplasm.  However,  such  an  escape  from 
frog  nerve,  though  often  investigated,  is  doubtful,  except  following 
very  prolonged  stimulation  (for  example,  60  stimuli  per  second,  for 
1-3  hours,  in  the  experiments  of  Arnett  and  Wilde,  with  Fenn).^^ 
However,  this  may  be  accounted  for,  by  assuming  that  only  a  very 
small  area  of  the  surface  of  a  myelinated  nerve,  the  Ranvier  nodes, 
is  available  for  diffusion.  This  can  be  correlated  with  the  experiments 
of  Cole  and  Curtis, ^^  regarding  impedance  and  membrane  capacity 
of  the  squid  nerve.  Notwithstanding  the  fact  that,  during  excitation, 
the  resistance  of  the  squid  nerve  falls  off  from  1000  ohm/square-cm. 
to  only  25  ohm/square-cm.,  not  more  than  2%  of  the  area  is  involved 
in  the  increase  of  permeability.  This  means  that  the  remainder, 
about  98%,  would  be  inactive.  Another  point  is  the  fact  that  the 
state  of  excitation,  in  general,  lasts  only  a  very  short  time,  measured 
in  milliseconds.  Very  slowly  reacting  cells,  therefore,  may  offer  a 
greater  chance  to  detect  an  ion  escape.  As  a  matter  of  fact,  the  con- 
ductivity of  the  water  on  the  outside  of  the  surface  of  a  Nitella  cell 
rises  perceptibly,  after  several  excitation  waves  have  passed  the  slowly 
responding  object,  the  excitation  time  being  measured  in  tenths  of  a 
second  (Cole  and  Curtis).  Since  depolarization  is  followed  by  re- 
polarization, the  question  arises,  whether  and  how  the  ions  which 
escape  through  the  leaky  membrane  are  recovered.  It  becomes  in- 
creasingly clear  that,  in  one  way  or  the  other,  energy  is  utilized  for 
this  purpose.  In  other  words,  the  physiological  membranes  are  more 
than  labile  structures.    Rather,  they  are,  or  can  be,  acting  machineries. 


HOBER:  THE  MEMBRANE  THEORY 


387 


For  example:  According  to  Furusawa,  Feng,  and  Shanes  and  Brown/^ 
during  anoxia  the  polarization  of  crab  nerve  and  its  excitability  fall 
off  reversibly,  but  seem  to  be  maintained  in  the  presence  of  phos- 
phopyruvic  acid,  adenosintriphosphate,  and  thiamin;  in  other  words, 
by  establishing  the  normal  glycolytic  cycle.  According  to  Hoagland 
and  Davis,^°  Nitella  cells  in  the  dark  lose  their  intracellular  chloride 
ions,  through  the  protoplasmic  wall,  into  the  surrounding  water  and 
recapture  them  during  exposure  to  light.  Furthermore,  according  to 
J.  E.  Harris, ^^  potassium  ion  gets  lost  from  human  erythrocytes  at  low 
temperature,  but  re-enters  at  room  temperature,  after  addition  of 
glucose. 

REVERSAL  OF  THE  NORMAL  ACTION  POTENTIAL 

I  now  come  back  to  the  lately-discovered  fact,  already  mentioned, 
that  the  potential  change  during  action  does  not  equal  the  resting 
potential  in  magnitude,  as  it  was  assumed  for  many  years.  Rather, 
by  overshooting  the  zero  line,  as  shown  in  figure  1,  the  potential  is 


Figure  1.  Potential  of  the  internal  electrode.  The  figure  shows  that  the  resting  potential  is 
— 44  mV.  During  activity,  the  potential  overshoots  to  the  positive  side,  +40  mV,  so  that  the 
action  potential  wave  amounts  to  84  mV.     ( Hodgrkin  &  Huxley^.) 

momentarily  reversed  in  sign,  the  outside  of  the  membrane  becoming 
negative  to  the  inside.  This  reversal  during  passage  of  the  impulse 
does  not  fit  into  the  classical  picture  of  the  behavior  of  the  active  nerve 
membrane,  and  possibly  indicates  a  special  mechanism,  which  is  super- 
imposed to  the  mechanism  of  the  customary  excitation  depolarization. 
Figure  2  depicts  three  conditionsof  the  nerve  membrane:  (a)  represents 
the  normal  polarization  of  a  resting  nerve  membrane;  (b)  is  indicative 
of  the  depolarized  membrane,  which,  according  to  the  ordinary  view- 


388  ANNALS  NEW  YORK  ACADEMY  OF  SCIENCES 

point,  during  activity  is  fairly  equally  permeable  to  cation  and  anion, 
to  this  extent  resembling  the  situation  in  injury;  while,  in  (c),  an 
additional  influence  of  organic  anions  has  been  taken  into  account, 
as  has  been  discussed  already  by  Hodgkin  and  Huxley.-    These  authors 

d-,,.<j  V.S+  _± 1 1 1 ± 1 1_      -its  nV        (a) 

^"'1  -r-)        ::  ::  ::  ::  ::  ::  ;       01^    (b) 

a/opkt>n. 


^--',^'--        ::l::i::t::i::t::i^   ^-fow    (c) 

Figure  2.    Diagrams  illustrating  the  reversal  of  the  nerve  potential. 

have  considered,  among  others,  particularly  the  lactic  acid  anion,  which, 
during  activity,  would  penetrate  the  membrane  from  inside  and  pro- 
duce a  negativity  outside.  However,  this  hypothesis  is  rejected  by 
Hodgkin  and  Huxley  themselves,  as  it  would  be  hard  to  imagine  the 
concentration  and  the  mobility  in  the  membrane  of  the  lactate  ion  as 
being  sufficient.  Instead,  I  would  prefer  to  pay  attention,  especially, 
to  the  organic  nonpolar-polar,  hydrophobic-hydrophilic  anions,  already 
mentioned,  which  possibly  can  be  assumed  to  be  present  in  the  nerve 
membrane,  or,  rather,  to  be  liberated  as  the  excitation  wave  travels 
along  the  fiber.  As  stated  earlier,  such  anions,  locally  applied  to  the 
outside  of  a  muscle,  call  forth  a  reversed  resting  potential,  whereas, 
if  they  originated  during  excitation  inside,  they  would  call  forth  a 
reversed  action  potential,  due  to  the  fact  that  the  adsorption  forces 
would  draw  these  anions  into  the  porous  membrane,  as  shown  in  figure 
2(c).  Such  a  reversed  resting  potential  has  been  found  with  the  salts 
of  higher  fatty  acids,  alkyl  sulfates,  aryl  sulfonates,  and  others.  These 
experiments  should  be  extended  to  nerves,  especially  to  single  nerve 
fibers  like  that  of  the  squid,  for  the  following  reason: 

Nonpolar-polar  anions  are  abundantly  preformed  in  the  molecules  of 
lipoids  of  the  nervous  system,  chiefly  in  phospholipids  and  cerebrosides. 
Among  their  split  products,  the  nonpolar-polar  character  is  especially 
pronounced  in  the  anions  of  fatty  acids  with  long  carbon  atom  chains, 
and,  according  to  Langmuir  and  Adam,  particularly  in  fatty  acids 


HOBER:  THE  MEMBRANE  THEORY  389 

with  one  to  three  double  bonds,  e.g.,  in  oleic,  linolic,  linoleic,  arachi- 
donic,  nervonic,  and  oxynervonic  acids.  These  long  carbon  atom 
chains  of  the  lipoids,  lecithin,  kephalin,  sphingomyelin,  and  cerebrosides 
seem  to  be  existent,  not  only  in  the  massive  sheath  of  the  myelinated 
nerve  fibers,  but,  according  to  Young  and  Francis  Schmitt,^^  also  in 
the  unmyelinated  fibers  of  crabs  and  cephalopods  (for  instance,  the 
squid  nerve) ,  where  the  thickness  of  the  sheath  has  been  found  to  be 
as  small  as  one  per  cent  of  the  diameter  of  the  axon,  i.e.,  about  5/i, 
compared  to  25  per  cent  of  the  diameter  in  vertebrates,  as  shown  by 
Pumphrey  and  Young  (plate  1).  In  the  sheath  of  the  unmyelinated 
fibers,  the  lipoids,  though  often  not  demonstrable  by  the  customary 
staining  with  osmium  tetroxide,  can  be  detected  with  polarization 
optics  (Bear  and  F.  0.  Schmitt-^). 

Now,  looking  upon  the  excitatory  process  from  the  standpoint  of  the 
old  "Stromchen  theory"  of  Hermann,^*  it  is  at  the  boundary  between  the 
stimulated  altered  and  the  adjacent,  unaltered  region  that  small  local 
circuit  currents  arise,  flowing  out  of  the  unaltered  region,  which  then 
secondarily  gets  altered  as  in  a  catelectrotonus,  and  flowing  in  at  the 
originally  stimulated  region,  which,  thus,  is  inactivated  as  in  an  anelec- 
trotonus.  Catelectrotonus,  however,  as  mentioned  before,  means  soften- 
ing the  colloidal  membrane  and  dispersing  its  structural  aggregates  by 
way  of  potassium  and  chloride  ions  and  depolarization  of  the  normal 
resting  membrane.  Anelectrotonus,  on  the  other  hand,  means  condensa- 
tion and  re-polarization.  The  aforementioned  increase  of  concentration 
of  potassium,  which  happens  to  be  produced  in  the  membrane  by  the 
outflowing  current,  may  then  serve  to  liberate  in  the  nerve  membrane, 
directly,  some  of  the  nonpolar-polar  anions,  as,  according  to  the  well- 
known  studies  of  G.  L.  Brown  and  W.  Feldberg,"  acetylcholine  is  liber- 
ated by  even  a  very  small  surplus  of  potassium  (amounting  to  not  more 
than  0.01  per  cent)  in  the  perfused  ganglion  cells,  where  it  normally 
is  fixed  in  a  nondiffusible  state.  However,  the  mechanism  of  this  re- 
lease is  by  no  means  clearer  than  that  just  suggested  for  the  nonpolar- 
polar  anions.  Alternatively,  the  nonpolar-polar  anions  could  possibly 
be  liberated,  indirectly,  by  an  activation  of  lecithase  A,  an  enzyme 
occurring  in  nerve  tissue,  which  is  known  to  set  free  the  unsaturated, 
but  not  the  saturated,  fatty  acids  of  the  lipoid  molecules. ^^ 

These  are  speculations,  it  is  true.  If,  however,  we  refer  them  to  the 
giant  axon  of  the  cephalopods,  which  was  studied,  in  recent  years, 
with  most  diversified  and  modern  methods,  it  means  that  probably 
the  alterations  are  bound  up  with  the  thin  surface  membrane  which 
wraps  up  the  voluminous  column  of  axoplasm,  and  that  this  fine  mem- 


390  ANNALS  NEW  YORK  ACADEMY  OF  SCIENCES 

brane  would  be  the  site  of  a  complex  chemistry.  Although  the  dis- 
cussion of  the  chemical  side  of  nerve  activity  is  beyond  the  scope  of 
this  paper,  I  should  hke  to  conclude  by  turning  to  some  interesting 
observations  of  von  Muralt  (1942),  involving  the  appearance,  during 
excitation,  of  a  substance  which  may  bring  about  the  reversal  of  the 
membrane  polarization. 

When  an  excised  frog  sciatic  is  stimulated,  electrically,  at  a  certain 
frequency  and  simultaneously  is  dipped  with  a  certain  velocity  into 
liquid  air,  several  excitation  waves  must  be  caught  and  frozen  along 
the  nerve.  When  an  extract  of  stimulated  and  unstimulated  nerves, 
pulverized  in  the  frozen  state,  is  made  up  with  eserinized  frog  Ringer 
or  serum,  it  appears  that,  during  excitation,  the  nerve  has  liberated 
minute  amounts  of  several  substances.  One  of  these,  by  various  tests, 
is  identified  as  acetylcholine ;  and  a  second  substance  is  concentrated  in 
the  foam  of  the  extract,  which,  from  this  sign  of  surface  activity, 
possibly  indicates  the  presence  of  a  nonpolar-polar  substance,  whereas, 
in  the  foam  from  an  acetylcholine-eserin-serum  solution  as  a  control, 
the  acetylcholine  fails  to  show  an  accumulation.  Recently,  by  the 
same  freezing  method,  von  Muralt  has  intercepted  a  third  substance, 
thiamin,  which  possibl)^  also  is  surface-inactive.-^  Certainly,  these 
results  are  far  from  giving  conclusive  support  to  the  concept  that  non- 
polar-polar substances,  detectable  by  their  surface  activity,  have  been 
liberated  during  excitation.  Even  if  they  were,  the  liberation  may  be  of 
minor  significance,  considering  the  fact  that,  according  to  Hopkins  and 
Huxley,  and  to  Curtis  and  Cole,  the  resting  potentials  of  the  giant 
nerve  fibers  vary  little  from  one  experiment  to  another,  in  contrast  to 
a  wide  variability  appearing  in  the  size  of  their  action  potentials.  In 
any  case,  this  grouj)  of  observations  emphasizes  the  urgent  need  to 
extend  the  study  of  chemical  products,  which  are  directly  connected 
with  nerve  activity,  beyond  the  demonstration  of  acetylcholine. 

BIBLIOGRAPHY 

1.  Hbber,  R. 

1905.     Pfliig.  Arch.  ges.  Physiol.  106:  .599. 

2.  Hodgkin,  A.  L.,  &  A.  F.  Huxley 

1939.  Nature  144:  710. 
1945.     J.  Physiol.  104:  176. 

Webb,  D.  A.",  &  J.  Z.  Young 

1940.  .1.  Physiol.  98:  299. 

3.  Curtis,  H.  J.,  &  K.  S.  Cole 

1942.     .1.  Cell.  Comp.  Physiol.  19:  135. 

4.  Osterhout,  W.  V.,  et  al. 

1927.     J.  Gen.  Physiol.  11:  193. 


HOBKR:  THE  MEMBRANE  THEORY  391 

5.  Graham,  J.,  G.  R.  Carlson,  &  R.  W.  Gerard 

1942.     Fed.  Proe.  1:  31. 

6.  Hober,  R.,  M.  Andersch,  J.  Hober,  &  B.  Nebel 
1939.     J.  Cell.  Comp.  Physiol.  13:  195. 

7.  Hober,  R. 
Unpublished  Data. 

8.  Hober,  R. 

1917.     Pfliig-.  Arch.  ^es.  Physiol.  166:  531. 

9.  Bear,  R.  S.,  &  F.  O.  Schmitt 

1939.  J.  Cell.  Comp.  Physiol.  14:  205,  351. 

10.  Cowan,  S.  L. 

1934.     Proc.  Roy.  Soc.  London  B  115:  216. 

11.  Fenn,  W.  O.,  et  al. 

1934.     Am.  J.  Physiol.  110:  74. 

12.  Labes,  R.,  &  H.  Zain 

1926.  Arch.  E.xp.  Path.  Pharm.  125. 

1927.  .Vrch.  Exp.  Path.  Pharm.  126. 

13.  Ebbecke,  U. 

1933.     Ergebn.  Physiol.  35:  756. 

1922.  Pfliig.  Arch',  ges.  Physiol.  195:  555. 

14.  Blinks,  L.  R. 

1936.     .J.  Gen.  Physiol.  20:  229. 

15.  Woronzow,  D.  S. 

1924-1929.     Pfliig.  Arch.  ges.  Physiol.  203,  207,  210,  216,  223. 
Hober,  R.,  &  H.  Strobe 

1929.     Pfliig.  Arch.  ges.  Physiol.  222:  71. 

16.  Guttman,  R.,  &  K,  S.  Cole 

1941.     Proc.  Soc.  Exp.  Biol.  &  Med.  48:  293. 
Guttman,  R. 

1940.  .J.  Gen.  Physiol.  23:343. 

17.  Arnett,  V.,  &  S.  Wilde 

1941.  J.  Xeurophysiol.  4:  572. 

18.  Cole,  K.  S.,  &  H.  J.  Curtis 
1939.     J.  Gen.  Physiol.  22:  649. 

19.  Furusawa,  K. 

1929.     J.  Physiol.  67:  325. 
Feng,  T.  P. 

19.32.     J.  Physiol.  76:477. 
Shanes,  A.  M.,  &  D.  E.  S.  Brown 

1942.  J.  Cell.  Comp.  Physiol.  19:  1. 

20.  Hoagland,  D.  R.,  &  A.  R.  Davis 

1923.  J.  Gen.  Physiol.  5:  629. 

21.  Harris,  J.  E. 

1941.     J.  Biol.  Chem.  141:579. 

22.  Young,  J.  Z. 

1936.     Proc.  Roy.  Soc.  London  B  121:  319. 
Schmitt,  F.  O. 

1936.  Cold  Spring  Harbor  Sympos.  4:  7. 
Pumphrey,  R.  J.,  &  J.  Z.  ifoung 
1938.     J.  Exp.  Biol.  15:433. 

23.  Bear,  A.  S.,  &  F.  O.  Schmitt 

1937.  J.  CeU.  Comp.  Physiol.  9:  275. 
Young,  J.  Z. 

1933.     Cold  Spring  Harbor  Sympos.  4:1. 

24.  Lillie,  R.  S. 

1923.     Protoplasmic  Action  and  Nerve  Action.    Chicago  Univ.  Press.    Chicago, 
Illinois. 


392  ANNALS  NEW  YORK  ACADEMY  OF  SCIENCES 

25.  Brown,  G.  L.,  &  W.  Feldberg 

1936.     J.  Physiol.  83:  290. 

26.  Belfanti,  S.,  A.  Contardi,  &  A.  Ercoli 
1936.     Erg.  Enzymforsch.  6:  213. 

Schmidt,  G.,  B.  Hershman,  &  S.  J.  Thannha3uer 
1945.     J.  Biol.  Chem.  161:  523. 

27.  von  Muralt,  A. 

1942.  Pflug.  Arch.  ges.  Physiol.  245:  604. 

28.  Liechti,  A.,  A.  von  Muralt,  &  M.  Rsinsrt 

1943.  Hslvet.  Physiol.  Acta.  1:  79. 
von  Muralt,  A. 

1945.     Experientia  1(5). 


IIOBER:  THE  MEMBRANE  THEORY  393 


PLATE  1 


394  ANNALS  NEW  YORK  ACADEMY  OF  SCIENCES 


Plate  1 
Large  axon  and  small  axons  of  stellar  nerve  of  Sepia  officinalis.     (Young.^^) 


Annals  N.  Y.  Acad.  Sn. 


Vol..    XI.VII.    AkT.    4.    Pl.xTK    1 


I. ax 


sax 


HOBKR:  THE   MEMBRAXK  THEORY 


CHEMICAL  MECHANISM  OF  NERVE  ACTIVITY 

By  David  Nachmansohn* 

Department  oj  Neurology,  College  of  Physicians  and  Surgeons,  Columbia  Uni- 
versity, New  York,  N.  Y. 

INTRODUCTION 

The  electrical  signs  of  nervous  action  were,  for  a  century,  the  only 
manifestations  studied  by  neurophysiologists.  But  the  function  of  a 
living  cell  cannot  be  conceived  in  purely  physical  terms.  This  was 
clearly  expressed  by  Gasser,  when  he  compared  the  electric  spikes  to 
the  ticks  of  the  clock,  both  being  only  signs  of  activity.^  For  a  thor- 
ough understanding  of  the  mechanism  of  nerve  activity,  a  knowledge 
of  the  chemical  reactions  involved  is  essential.  Biophysics  and  bio- 
chemistry are,  consequently,  of  equal  importance  and  inseparable  in 
any  attempt  to  solve  the  problem. 

The  special  function  of  the  nervous  system  is  that  of  carrying  mes- 
sages from  one  distant  point  of  the  body  to  another.  This  process 
may  be  subdivided  into  three  successive  phases:  First,  a  stimulus 
reaching  a  neuron  has  to  initiate  an  impulse.  Second,  the  impulse  once 
initiated  has  to  be  propagated  along  the  axon.  Finally,  the  impulse 
arriving  at  the  nerve  ending  has  to  be  transmitted  either  to  a  second 
neuron  or  to  an  effector  cell.  Early  in  this  century,  T.  R.  Elliot 
had  the  idea  that  the  third  phase,  namely,  the  transmission  of 
the  nerve  impulse  from  the  nerve  ending  to  the  effector  cell,  may  be 
carried  out  by  a  chemical  compound  released  from  the  nerve  ending  and 
acting  directly  on  the  second  unit.  Elliot  suggested  that  adrenaline 
may  be  the  transmitter  of  the  impulse  from  the  sympathetic  nerve  end- 
ing to  the  effector  cell.-  He  based  this  idea  on  the  similarity  between 
the  action  of  adrenaline  and  the  effect  of  stimulation  of  sympathetic 
nerves  on  the  effector  organ.  Similar  ideas  were  advanced  subsequently 
by  Dixon  and  Howell. 

In  1921,  Otto  Loewi  found  that,  following  vagus  stimulation  of  an 
isolated  frog's  heart,  a  compound  appeared  in  the  perfusion  fluid  which, 
when  transmitted  to  a  second  heart,  produced  an  effect  similar  to  that 
of  vagus  stimulation.  Accepting  the  basic  idea  of  Elliot,  Otto  Loewi 
concluded  that  this  compound,  which  was  later  identified  with  acetyl- 


*  Most  of  the  work  described  in  this  lecture  has  been  supported  by  grants  from  the  Josiah  Macy, 
Jr.,  Foundation  and  the  Dazian  Foundation  for  Medical  Research. 

(395) 


396  ANNALS  NEW  YORK  ACADEMY  OF  SCIENCES 

choline  (ACh),  is  actually  released  from  the  nerve  ending  and  acts  on 
the  heart  cell  directly.^  The  concept  of  "neurohumoral"  transmission 
appeared  enlightening  in  the  case  of  the  autonomic  nerves  and  was 
widely  accepted  among  physiologists. 

In  1933,  Dale  tried  to  extend  this  idea  of  a  "chemical  mediator"  of 
the  nerve  impulse  to  the  neuromuscular  junction  and  to  the  ganglionic 
synapse.*  In  this  case,  however,  the  theory  encountered  strong  opposi- 
tion. In  addition  to  many  contradictions  and  difficulties,  summarized  by 
Eccles,^  there  were  two  main  objections.  The  first  was  the  time  factor. 
The  transmission  of  nerve  impulses  across  neuromuscular  junctions 
and  ganglionic  synapses  occurs  in  milliseconds.  No  evidence  was 
available  that  the  chemical  process  can  occur  at  the  high  speed  re- 
quired, and  Dale  admitted  this  difficulty.  The  second  objection  was 
still  more  fundamental.  According  to  leading  neurophysiologists,  the 
excitable  properties  of  axon  and  cell  body  are  basically  the  same.  The 
electric  signs  of  nervous  action  do  not  support  the  assumption  that  the 
transmission  of  the  nerve  impulse  along  the  axon  differs,  fundamentally, 
from  that  across  the  synapse. 

The  idea  of  a  chemical' mediator,  released  at  the  nerve  ending  and 
acting  directly  on  the  second  neuron,  thus  appeared  to  be  unsatisfac- 
tory in  many  respects  (Fulton'') . 

NEW  APPROACH 

Recognition  of  two  features  of  nervous  action  is  essential  to  an  under- 
standing of  the  problems  and  the  difficulties  involved:  The  high  speed 
of  the  propagation  of  the  impulse,  and  the  smallness  of  the  energy  re- 
quired. In  medullated  mammahan  nerve,  the  impulse  travels  at  the 
rate  of  100  meters  per  second,  and  the  energy  required  per  impulse  per 
gram  is  less  than  1/10  of  a  millionth  of  a  small  calorie.  The  recording 
of  such  an  event  offers  many  difficulties,  even  with  the  use  of  specialized 
physical  methods.  Only  in  the  last  twenty  years  have  really  adequate 
instruments  become  available  for  the  analysis  of  physical  aspects  of 
nervous  function. 

It  is  obvious  that  the  study  of  the  chemical  reactions  connected  with 
an  event  of  this  kind  must  offer  even  more  serious  difficulties.  No  ade- 
quate methods  are  available  for  directly  determining  chemical  com- 
pounds appearing  in  such  minute  amounts  and  for  such  short  periods 
of  time.  There  is,  however,  another  possible  approach.  Nearly  all 
chemical  reactions  in  the  living  cell  are  effectuated  by  enzymes.  The 
study  of  enzymes  in  vitro  has  elucidated  many  chemical  reactions. 


NACHMANSOHN:  CHEMICAL  MECHANISM  IN  NERVES        397 

known  to  occur  in  living  cells,  which  could  not  be  followed  by  direct 
chemical  determination  of  the  compounds  metabolized.  Especially  for 
an  event  occurring  with  such  a  high  speed  as  the  propagation  of  the 
nerve  impulse,  analysis  of  the  enzyme  systems  involved  appeared  to  be 
the  most  promising  approach. 

Enzyme  studies  alone  are,  however,  not  sufficient  for  the  elucidation 
of  a  biological  mechanism,  since  there  are  so  many  simultaneous  enzy- 
matic reactions  in  the  complex  system  of  the  living  cell.  It  is  necessary 
to  correlate  enzyme  activities  with  events  in  the  intact  cell  recorded 
by  physical  methods.  The  most  conspicuous  example  of  such  an  ap- 
proach is  the  development  of  muscle  physiology.  Through  the  pioneer 
work  of  A.  V.  Hill  and  0.  Meyerhof,  many  physical  and  chemical 
changes  have  been  correlated,  and  our  concept  of  the  mechanism  of 
muscular  contraction  has,  according  to  an  expression  of  A.  V.  Hill, 
gone  through  a  real  "revolution." 

The  question  of  the  role  of  ACh  in  the  mechanism  of  nerve  activity 
has  been  approached  by  the  study  of  the  enzyme  systems  involved  in 
the  formation  and  hydrolysis  of  the  ester.  On  the  basis  of  their  be- 
havior in  vitro,  the  activities  of  the  enzymes  could  be  correlated  in  dif- 
ferent ways  with  events  in  the  living  cell  recorded  by  physical  methods. 
The  facts  established  show  that  the  original  theories  of  the  role  of  ACh, 
and,  more  generally,  the  idea  of  "chemical  mediation,"  have  to  be  mod- 
ified. There  is  a  strong  body  of  evidence  that  the  release  and  re- 
moval of  ACh  is  an  intracellular  process,  occurring  at  points  along  the 
neuronal  surface  and  directly  associated  with  the  nerve  action  poten- 
tial. The  agent,  however,  which  transmits  the  impulse  along  the  axon, 
as  well  as  across  the  synapse,  is  the  action  potential.*'"^  Some  of  the 
most  important  features  of  these  investigations  may  be  briefly  outlined. 

I.     CHOLINESTERASE 

A.     Time  Factor 

ACh  is  inactivated  by  the  enzyme  cholinesterase,  which  hydrolyzes 
the  ester  into  choline  and  acetic  acid.  The  first  essential  result  of  the 
studies  of  this  enzyme  has  been  the  evidence  of  its  high  concentration 
in  nerve  tissue:  Significant  amounts  of  ACh  may  be  split  in  milli- 
seconds; that  is,  a  period  of  time  of  the  order  required  for  the  passage 
of  a  nerve  impulse.  Consequently,  the  potential  rate  of  ACh  metab- 
olism is  thus  sufficiently  high  to  permit  the  assumption  that  it  parallels 
the  rate  of  the  electric  changes  and  may,  therefore,  be  directly  con- 
nected with  the  nerve  action  potential. 


398  ANNALS  NEW  YORK  ACADEMY  OF  SCIENCES 

The  special  case  in  which  this  problem  of  the  time  factor  has  been 
studied  and  received  a  satisfactory  answer,  is  the  frog's  sartorius  mus- 
cle.^°  A  small  fraction  of  this  muscle  is  free  of  nerve  endings.  By 
determining  the  concentration  of  cholinesterase  in  this  part  of  the 
muscle,  in  the  part  containing  nerve  endings,  and  in  the  nerve  fibers, 
it  is  possible  to  calculate  the  concentration  of  cholinesterase  at  the 
motor  end-plates.  Since  the  number  of  end-plates  in  a  frog's  sartorius 
is  known,  the  amount  of  ACh  which  may  be  split  during  one  milli- 
second at  a  single  motor  end-plate  can  be  calculated.  This  turns  out 
to  be  1.6  X  10"  molecules  of  the  ester.  About  one-third  of  the  enzyme 
at  the  motor  end-plate  appears  to  be  localized  inside  the  nerve  ending. 
On  the  assumption  that  one  molecule  of  ACh  covers  about  20-50 
square  A,  the  amount  which  may  be  hydrolyzed  during  one  millisecond 
at  one  end-plate  would  cover  a  surface  of  100-250  square  microns. 

A  high  concentration  of  cholinesterase,  of  an  order  of  magnitude 
similar  to  that  at  motor  end-plates,  exists  at  all  synapses,  whether 
central  or  peripheral,  mammalian  or  fish,  vertebrate  or  invertebrate." 
In  mammalian  brain,  for  instance,  10^^  to  10^°  molecules  of  ACh  may 
be  activated  per  gram  of  tissue  during  one  millisecond.  This  corre- 
sponds to  about  10-100  millions  of  square  microns  of  neuronal  surface. 

These  experiments  removed  one  of  the  chief  difficulties  from 
the  theory  that  ACh  is  involved  in  the  transmission  of  nerve  im- 
pulses. They  established  that  the  ester  may  be  metabolized  at  the 
high  speed  required  for  a  chemical  reaction  directly  connected  with 
such  a  rapid  event. 

The  difference  between  synaptic  region  and  fiber  is,  however,  only 
quantitative.  The  concentration  of  cholinesterase  is  high  everywhere 
in  nerves,  although  it  rises  at  the  region  of  synapses. 

B.     Localization  of  Cholinesterase  at  the  Neuronal  Surface 

The  second  essential  feature  is  the  localization  of  cholinesterase  in 
the  neuronal  surface.  Direct  evidence  for  this  localization  has  been 
offered  with  experiments  on  the  giant  axon  of  squid  {Loligo  pealii^-) . 
This  axon  has  a  diameter  ranging  from  0.5  to  1.0  mm.  The  axoplasm 
may  be  extruded  and  thus  separated  from  the  envelope.  The  envelope 
is  formed  of  connective  tissue,  lipoid  and  plasma  membrane.  The 
axoplasm  was  found  to  be  practically  free  of  cholinesterase.  The  whole 
enzyme  activity  is  in  the  envelope. 

This  exclusive  localization  of  an  enzyme  in  the  neuronal  surface  has 
been  found  only  in  the  case  of  cholinesterase.  Respiratory  enzymes 
are  localized  nearly  completely  in  the  axoplasm.^''     Bioelectric  phe- 


NACHMANSOHN:  CHEMICAL  MECHANISM  IN  NERVES        399 

nomena  occur  at  the  surface.  The  high  concentration  of  the  enzyme 
at  the  surface  suggested  that  ACh  may  be  connected  with  conduction 
along  the  axon,  as  well  as  with  transmission  across  the  synapse.  This 
view  is  consistent  with  the  conclusion  of  neurophysiologists  that  the 
mechanism  of  these  two  events  is  fundamentally  the  same. 

II.     CORRELATION  BETWEEN  ENZYME  ACTIVITY  AND 
PHYSICAL  EVENTS  DURING  NERVOUS  FUNCTION 

The  high  rate  of  ACh  metabolism  and  the  locahzation  of  the  enzyme 
at  the  neuronal  surface  made  possible  the  assumption  that  the  ester  is 
connected  with  the  electrical  manifestations  of  nerve  activity.  How- 
ever, suggestive  as  these  facts  may  be,  observations  on  enzymes,  as 
pointed  out  before,  do  not  permit  an  interpretation  of  the  actual  role 
of  the  substrate.  For  an  understanding  of  the  precise  function  of  an 
enzyme,  its  activity  has  to  be  connected  with  events  in  the  living  cell 
which,  in  the  case  of  nerve,  can  only  be  recorded  by  physical  means. 
Such  a  relationship  has  been  established  in  three  different  ways. 

A.     Parallelism  Between  the  Voltage  of  the  Action  Potential  and 

Cholinesterase  Activity 

The  first  line  of  investigations  in  which  a  correlation  between  physi- 
cal and  chemical  processes  was  obtained,  was  in  experiments  on  the 
electric  fish.  It  was  found  that  the  activity  of  cholinesterase  in  the 
electric  organ  parallels  exactly  the  voltage  of  the  action  potential. 

The  powerful  electric  discharge  in  these  organs  is  identical  in  nature 
with  the  nerve  action  potential  of  ordinary  nerves  (A.  V.  HilP*).  The 
only  distinction  is  the  arrangement  of  the  nervous  elements,  the  elec- 
tric plates  in  series.  The  potential  difference  developed  by  a  single 
element  is  about  0.1  volt,  which  is  the  same  order  of  magnitude  as  that 
found  in  ordinary  nerves.  In  the  species  with  the  most  powerful  elec- 
tric organ  known,  Electrophonis  electricus,  the  so-called  electric  eel, 
several  thousand  elements  are  arranged  in  series  from  the  head  to  the 
caudal  end  of  the  organ.  Thus,  the  voltage  of  a  discharge  amounts 
to  400-600  volts,  on  the  average,  and,  in  some  specimens,  more  than 
800  volts  have  been  observed.  In  Torpedo,  another  species  with  a 
powerful  electric  organ,  the  elements  are  arranged  in  a  dorso-ventral 
direction.  Since  it  is  a  flat  fish,  the  number  of  plates  in  series  usually 
does  not  exceed  400  to  500,  and,  consequently,  the  discharge  is  only  30 
to  60  volts,  on  the  average. 

In  1937,  an  extraordinarily  high  concentration  of  cholinesterase  was 
found  in  the  strong  electric  organ  of  Torpedo.     In  the  following  year, 


400 


ANNALS  NEW  YORK  ACADEMY  OF  SCIENCES 


a  similar  high  concentration  was  found  in  the  electric  organ  of  Elec- 
trophorus  electricus.  The  organs,  in  one  hour,  hydrolyze  amounts  of 
ACh  equivalent  to  one  to  five  times  their  own  weight.  In  the  larger 
specimens,  the  organs  have  a  weight  of  several  kilograms,  so  that  the 


m. 


QCh.E.  V^ 
500- -J9^ 


400+ 15*2 


300--II.4 


200"  7.6 


100 


\ 


\     o 
\ 
\ 
\ 

V 


3.8 


0 
Head  end 


1— 
10 


20  30-^  cm. 

Caudal  end 


FiGUKB   1.    Action  potential   and   cholinesterase    activity  in  the  electric  organ,   specimen   no.    1. 
Length  of  fish,  51  cm. 

Abscissae :  distance  from  the  anterior  end  of  the  organ  in  cm. 
Ordinates:  QCh.E.  and  V/cm. 
•  average  QCh.E.  from  a  single  piece  of  tissue. 
+  average  QCh.E.  values  from  pieces  of  the  same  section. 
■  V/cm. 

amount  of  ACh  which  may  be  split  in  these  organs  may  amount  to 
several  kilograms  per  hour  or  several  milligrams  in  one-thousandth  of 
a  second.  These  are  significant  amounts.  They  make  possible  the 
assumption  that  ACh  is  directly  connected  with  the  action  potential  and 
may  even  generate  it,  for,  in  this  case,  the  compound  must  appear  and 
disappear  in  milliseconds.  If  speculation  were  to  be  excluded,  the  only 
means  of  removing  this  compound  so  rapidly  would  be  by  enzymatic 
action.     The  high  concentration  of  a  specific  enzyme  appeared  partic- 


NACHMANSOHN:  CHEMICAL  iM  EC  HAN  ISM  IN  NERVES        401 


ularly  significant,  in  view  of  the  cliemical  composition  of  these  organs: 
They  contain  92  per  cent  of  water  and  only  2  per  cent  of  protein. 

In  the  weak  electric  organ  of  the  common  Ray,  the  concentration  is 
relatively  low.  If,  in  the  three  species  mentioned,  voltage  and  number 
of  plates  per  centimeter  are  compared  with  the  concentration  of  cholin- 
esterase,  a  close  relationship  becomes  obvious. ^^'  ^® 

A  more  detailed  analysis  has  been  carried  out  on  the  electric  organ 
of  Electrophorus  electriciis.  This  species  is  particularly  favorable  for 
such  studies,  since  the  number  of  plates  per  centimeter,  and  conse- 
quently, the  voltage  per  centimeter,  decrease  from  the  head  to  the 
caudal  end  of  the  organ  (plates  2  and  3).     The  cholinesterase  activity 


200 


400 


acH  E 


Figure  2.    Correlation  between  voltage  and  cholinesterase  activity. 
The  voltage  per  cm.  is  plotted  against  the  enzyme  concentration.     The  dotted  line  is  calculated 
from  the  data  obtained  with  the  method  of  least  squares;   the  fully  drawn  line  calculated  on  the 
assumption  that  the  line  goes  through  the  0  point. 

decreases  in  the  same  proportion.  If  the  electric  changes  are  recorded 
and  compared  with  the  enzyme  activity  of  the  same  section,  a  close 
parallelism  is  obtained  between  voltage  and  enzyme  concentration 
(figure  1).  This  is  found  not  only  in  regard  to  the  variations  which 
occur  in  the  same  specimen,  but  even  for  the  variations  between  the 
individuals,  which  are  quite  considerable.^^ 

A  great  number  of  experiments  have  been  carried  out  on  fish  of 
various  sizes,  covering  a  range  of  the  action  potential  from  0.5  to 
22.0  volts  per  centimeter.  The  quotient  CH.E./V  was  found  to  be 
20.7,  with  a  standard  deviation  of  only  ±0.7  or  3.7  per  cent.  The 
standard  deviation  for  a  single  measurement  is  d=5.1  or  about  25  per 
cent.    This  is  good  uniformity  for  a  quotient  correlating  physical  and 


402  ANNALS  NEW  YORK  ACADEMY  OF  SCIENCES 

chemical  data.  Of  particular  importance  is  the  fact  that  the  line 
correlating  the  two  variables  apparently  goes  through  0  (figure  2). 
This  indicates  a  direct  proportionality.  The  results  are  consistent 
with  the  concept  that  the  physical  and  chemical  processes  recorded  are 
directly  associated  and,  consequently,  interdependent.^^  Such  a  par- 
allelism has  not  been  found  with  other  compounds  or  enzyme  activities 
known  to  be  connected  with  nervous  action. 

The  direct  proportionality  found  between  physical  and  ch,emical 
events  is  significant,  in  view  of  the  changing  morphological  structure 
of  the  electric  unit,  the  electric  plate.  If  all  plates  were  identical  in 
structure,  as  e.g.,  in  the  case  of  the  electric  organ  of  Torpedo,  the  volt- 
age and  the  cholinesterase  activity  would  be  expected  to  be  directly 
proportional  to  the  number  of  plates.  The  situation  is  entirely  dif- 
ferent in  the  electric  organ  of  Electrophorus  electricus,  because  the 
structure  of  the  plates  shows  enormous  variations.  In  spite  of  all 
variations  of  the  visible  structure,  the  voltage  of  each  plate  is  the 
same,  namely,  close  to  100  millivolts.  It  has,  therefore,  to  be  assumed 
that  the  "active  membrane,"  with  which  the  electric  manifestations 
are  connected  and  which  is  not  yet  well  defined,  does  not  change,  but 
is  similar  in  all  plates.  The  direct  proportionality  found  between 
voltage  and  enzyme  activity  suggests,  then,  that  the  physical  and  chem- 
ical events  may  be  associated  with  the  same  membrane  and  that  they 
may  be  functionally  interdependent.  Here  again,  the  fact  is  important 
only  in  connection  with  the  great  number  of  other  observations,  espe- 
cially the  extraordinarily  high  speed  of  the  chemical  process,  without 
which  the  correlation  observed  would  not  have  the  same  interest. 

Two  assumptions  appear  possible  concerning  the  manner  in  which 
ACh  may  act:  It  may  produce  electromotive  force  directly  by  action 
on  the  surface,  or  it  may  decrease  the  resistance  by  increasing  the 
permeability  of  the  boundary.  Resistance  and  electromotive  force 
are  closely  related  properties.  So  far,  the  evidence  from  experiments 
on  nerves  is  in  favor  of  a  change  in  resistance  and  increased  perme- 
ability. On  the  basis  of  alternating  current  impedance  measurements 
carried  out  on  the  giant  axon  of  squid,  Cole  and  Curtis  calculated  that 
the  resistance  drops  during  the  passage  of  the  impulse  from  1,000  ohms 
to  about  25  ohms  per  square  centimeter.^^  In  experiments  on  the  elec- 
tric tissue,  a  comparable  drop  in  resistance  was  found  by  Cox,  Coates, 
and  Brown. 2°  There  is  no  conclusive  evidence  that  electromotive  force 
is  actually  produced  during  the  passage  of  the  impulse.  One  possible 
interpretation  on  the  basis  of  the  material  available  at  present  is,  there- 
fore, the  assumption  that  the  parallelism  found  between  voltage  and 


NACHMANSOHN:  CHEMICAL  MECHANISM  IN  NERVES        403 

ACh  metabolism  may  be  due  essentially  to  the  effect  of  the  ester  on  the 
resistance  of  the  boundary  or,  which  is  equivalent,  on  its  permeability. 

Thus,  we  arrive  at  the  following  picture  of  the  role  which  ACh  may 
have  in  the  mechanism  of  nerve  activity :  According  to  the  membrane 
theory  which  is  most  widely  accepted  among  physiologists,  the  nerve  is 
surrounded  by  a  polarized  membrane.  The  polarized  state  of  the 
membrane  is  due  to  a  selective  permeability  to  potassium  ions  which 
are  many  times  more  concentrated  inside  the  axon  than  outside.  Dur- 
ing the  passage  of  the  impulse,  the  permeability  of  the  membrane  to 
negative  ions  is  increased,  and  a  depolarization  occurs.  The  rapid  ap- 
pearance and  removal  of  ACh  may  be  an  event  essential  for  this  change 
in  permeability.  The  depolarized  point  becomes  negative  to  the  adja- 
cent region,  and  flow  of  current  results.  This  flow  of  current  stimu- 
lates the  next  following  point.  There  again,  ACh  is  released,  and  the 
whole  process  repeated.  The  impulse  is  thus  propagated  along  the 
axon.  Cholinesterase  destroys  the  active  ester  very  rapidly,  and  the 
state  of  polarization  may  hereby  be  restored. 

At  the  nerve  ending,  other  factors,  like  increased  surface  and  de- 
creased resistance  leading  to  a  greater  flow  of  current,  may  act  in  ad- 
dition. But  the  process  is  fundamentally  identical,  the  transmitting 
agent  being  the  flow  of  current.  Whereas,  in  earlier  theories,  ACh 
was  considered  as  a  "neurohumoral"  or  "synaptic"  transmitter,  i.e.,  a 
substance  released  from  the  nerve  ending  and  acting  directly  on  a  sec- 
ond neuron,  in  the  new  concept  it  is  assumed  that  the  transmitting 
agent  is  always  the  electric  current,  the  action  potential,  but  the  re- 
lease of  ACh  is  necessary  for  generating  the  current. 

The  picture  is  consistent  with  the  idea  of  the  propagation  of  the 
nerve  impulse  as  developed  by  Keith  Lucas  and  Adrian.  It  becomes 
unnecessary  to  assume  that  the  transmission  along  the  axon  differs  fun- 
damentally from  that  across  the  synapse.  The  assumption  of  a  special 
mechanism  at  the  synapse,  different  from  that  in  the  axon,  as  empha- 
sized before,  was  the  chief  difficulty  which  had  to  be  overcome  to 
reconcile  the  original  theory  with  the  conclusions  of  the  electrophysi- 
ologists.  This  appeared  necessary  for  any  satisfactory  answer  to  the 
problem.  If  it  is  true  that  physical  methods  alone  are  unable  to  ex- 
plain the  mechanism  in  a  living  cell,  it  is  equally  true  that  conclusions 
based  on  chemical  methods  should  not  be  in  contradiction  to  those  ob- 
tained with  physical  methods,  in  view  of  the  much  higher  sensitivity 
of  the  latter. 

The  picture  of  the  transmission  of  the  nerve  impulse  across  the  syn- 
apse is,  however,  far  from  being  complete,  if  only  the  flow  of  current 


404  ANNALS  NEW  YORK  ACADEMY  OF  SCIENCES 

from  the  nerve  ending  to  the  second  unit  is  considered.  The  observa- 
tions of  Eccles  and  his  associates  have  shown  that  the  electric  current 
set  up  by  the  pre-synaptic  impulse  initiates  in  the  post-synaptic  mem- 
brane a  special  junctional  potential  (end-plate  potential  or,  more  gen- 
erally, synaptic  potential-^). 

These  findings  have  recently  found  a  morphological  correlate  by  the 
discovery  of  Couteaux  that  the  sarcoplasm  surrounding  the  presynap- 
tic nerve  ending  has  a  very  peculiar  structure.^^  It  is  similar  to  that 
described  by  several  authors  of  the  last  century  in  the  electroplasm 
which  surrounds  the  nerve  endings  in  the  electric  plates  of  electric  fish 
and  which  shows  a  layer  of  "electric  rods,"  the  "palisades"  of  Remak, 
at  that  particular  point. 

The  biochemical  data  support  the  assumption  of  a  high  rate  of 
ACh  metabolism  in  the  post-synaptic  membrane  of  the  neuro-muscular 
junction.  At  the  motor  end-plate  of  guinea  pig  gastrocnemius,  only 
one  third  of  the  cholinesterase  was  found  to  disappear  within  three  to 
four  weeks  after  section  of  the  motor  nerve.^^-  ^^  The  rest  remained 
there  for  many  months,  a  long  time  after  the  end-plate  had  been  trans- 
formed into  a  sole  plate.  It  appears,  thus,  probable  that  part  of  the 
high  concentration  of  cholinesterase  observed  at  the  motor  end-plate 
may  be  located  at  the  post-synaptic  membrane.  The  observations  on 
the  electric  organ  support  the  assumption  of  such  a  localization.  The 
electric  plates  which  form  the  electric  organ  are  homologous  to  motor 
end-plates.  The  discharge  in  these  organs  can  be  considered  as  com- 
parable to  the  end-plate  potential,  that  is,  a  response  of  the  post-synap- 
tic membrane.  The  direct  proportionality  found  between  the  voltage  of 
the  discharge  and  the  cholinesterase  activity  is,  therefore,  another  in- 
dication for  the  importance  of  ACh  in  the  post-synaptic  membrane. 

Specificity  of  Cholinesterase 

In  all  the  experiments  on  the  activity  of  the  enzyme,  it  was  assumed 
that  cholinesterase  is  specific  for  ACh.  In  such  a  case,  not  only  is  the 
conclusion  justified  that  the  substrate  metabolized  is  ACh,  but  also, 
the  activity  of  a  specific  enzyme  determined  in  vitro  may  well  be  used 
as  an  indication  for  the  potential  rate  of  metabolism  of  the  substrate  oc- 
curring in  vivo. 

It  appeared  imperative,  therefore,  to  demonstrate  the  specificity  of 
the  enzyme  for  ACh  in  all  those  tissues  which  were  used  in  the  inves- 
tigations leading  to  the  new  concept.  The  ester  linkage  in  ACh  shows 
no  peculiar  properties.  It  has,  therefore,  to  be  expected  that  the  ester 
can  be  hydrolyzed  by  other  esterases  and,  on  the  other  hand,  that 


NACHMANSOHN:  CHEMICAL  MECHANISM  IN  NERVES        405 

cholinesterase  can  hydrolyze  other  esters.  Specificity,  in  this  case, 
would  be  expected  on  the  basis  of  analogy  to  be  only  relative,  not 
absolute:  Cholinesterase  might  be  expected  to  split  ACh  at  a  higher 
rate  than  other  esters,  whereas  other  esterases  might  be  expected  to  be- 
have differently.  By  testing  a  number  of  substrates,  a  pattern  has 
been  obtained  which  makes  it  possible  to  distinguish  specific  cholin- 
esterase from  other  esterases. ^^ 

In  the  variety  of  nerve  tissues  which  have  been  used  as  basis  for 
establishing  the  new  concept,  the  enzyme  was  found  to  be  an  esterase 
specific  for  ACh:  viz.,  mammalian  brain,  lobster  nerve,  squid  fiber  con- 


1001- 


lOO      75       2S      3 


I  16 


NUC 
CAUD 

OX 


ACH     PRO    But    me      ben  trib    meBU 


^00 


a  00 


I 


KIDNEV 

GUINEA 
PIG 


100     2m  371        6        200     108      28 

Figure  3.  Pattern  of  cholinesterase  (nucleus  caudatus  of  ox)  in  presence  of  different  substrates 
compared  to  that  of  an  esterase  (kidney)  not  specific  for  acetylcholine. 

The  columns  represent  the  Q  of  the  substrates,  the  Q  of  ACh  being  100.  Abbreviations:  Pr  = 
propionylcholine,  Bu  =  butyrylrholine,  Me  =  acetyl -j8 -methyl choline  (mecholyi),  Be  =  benzoyl- 
choline,  Tr  =  tributyrin,  Mb  =  methyl  butyrate. 

taining  the  giant  axon,  and  the  electric  tissue.  All  show  a  similar 
pattern,  typical  for  cholinesterase.  Even  then,  rigid  statements  should 
be  avoided.  Occasional  deviations  in  one  or  the  other  directions  may 
be  expected.  Recent  observations  of  Richards  and  Cutcomp^*'  have 
revealed  that  the  cholinesterase  of  bee  brain  splits  acetyl-/3-methyl- 
choline  at  a  higher  rate  than  ACh,  whereas,  otherwise,  the  pattern  was 
typical  for  cholinesterase.  In  contrast,  the  hydrolysis  patterns  of  the 
esterase  of  other  organs  (liver,  kidney,  and  pancreas)  differ  greatly 
from  that  of  cholinesterase  (figure  3).  The  esterase  in  these  tissues 
shows  several  variations,  but  this  could  be  expected,  since  the  physio- 
logical substrate  is  unknown,  and  probably  varies  in  the  different  or- 
gans. They  should  be  referred  to  as  unspecified,  not  as  unspecific, 
esterases,  because  they  may  well  be  specific  for  substrates  not  as  yet 
specified.     ACh  is  metabolized  at  a  high  rate  only  in  nerve  tissue, 


406 


ANNALS  NEW  YORK  ACADEMY  OF  SCIENCES 


since  only  there  is  choline  acetylase  found.  If  the  esterase  in  all  nerve 
tissue  shows  a  pattern  so  distinctly  different  from  that  of  the  esterases 
of  other  tissues,  it  is  justifiable  to  consider  this  enzyme  as  a  specific 
cholinesterase. 


HOMOGENIZED 
ACH      PR        BU       ME       BE       TR. 


HDD 


M.B. 


100  97 


22  0  0 

M400 


IIDQ 


100         101  3  31  0  0 

1  =  3.000 


IIDQ 


100        109  1  18  0  0 

1:78,000 


HDD 


100        109  0  26  0  0  0 

Figure  4.  Rate  of  hydrolysis  of  different  esters  by  the  cholinesterase  of  the  electric  organ  of 
Electrophorus  electricus. 

The  first  row  gives  the  data  obtained  with  a  homogenized  suspension  of  electric  tissue.  In  such 
suspensions,  1  mg.  of  protein  splits  about  20-40  mgs.  of  ACh  per  hour. 

The  three  following  rows  show  the  data  obtained  with  increasing  degrees  of  purity,  1  ing.  of 
protein  splitting  1,400,  3,000,  and  78,000  of  ACh  per  hour,  respectively. 

Of  particular  importance  is  the  question  of  the  enzyme  present  in  the 
electric  tissue.  The  interpretation  given  for  the  direct  proportionality 
between  voltage  and  enzyme  activity  is  justified  only  if  the  enzyme  is 
exclusively,  or  almost  exclusively,  specific  cholinesterase.  Only  in 
that  case  can  the  proportionality  be  referred  to  as  an  interdependence 
between  ACh  metabolism  and  electric  manifestations. 

The  enzyme  extracted  from  the  electric  organ  of  Electrophorus  elec- 
tricus has  been  purified  by  fractional  ammonium  sulfate  precipitation. 
A  high  degree  of  purity  may  be  obtained  in  this  way.  1  milligram 
of  protein  is  capable  of  splitting  twenty  to  thirty  thousand  milligrams 
of  ACh  per  hour.  By  further  separation  of  the  proteins  by  high  speed 
centrifugation  (ultracentrifuge) ,  in  collaboration  with  Dr.  K.  G,  Stern, 
a  degree  of  purity  has  been  obtained  where  1  milligram  of  protein  was 
able  to  split  eighty  thousand  milligrams  of  ACh  per  hour.     If  the  rates 


NACHMANSOHN:  CHEMICAL  MECHANISM  IN  NERVES        407 

of  hydrolysis  of  different  substrates  by  the  highly  purified  enzyme  are 
compared  to  those  obtained  with  the  homogenized  suspension  of  electric 
tissue,  the  pattern  obtained  remains  exactly  the  same  throughout  the 
whole  process  of  purification  (figure  4).  Both  have  the  pattern  char- 
acteristic for  cholinesterase.  Thus,  the  correlation  established  between 
voltage  and  enzyme  activity  can  be  consequently  referred  to  a  corre- 
lation between  ACh  metabolized  and  voltage  developed. 

It  may  be  noted  that  examination  in  the  analytical  ultracentrifuge  in- 
dicates that  the  enzyme  is  a  very  large  molecule.  These  are  not  yet 
final  observations.  If  they  could  be  confirmed,  they  would  indicate 
that  the  turnover  number  of  the  enzyme  is  many  milhons  per  minute 
and  that  one  molecule  of  cholinesterase  could  split  one  molecule  of 
ACh  within  a  few  microseconds. 

B.     The  Energy  Source  of  the  Nerve  Action  Potential 

The  second  line  of  investigations,  in  which  enzyme  activity  could  be 
correlated  with  events  in  the  living  cell  recorded  by  physical  methods, 
is  based  on  the  energy  transformations  involved  and  on  thermodynamic 
considerations. 

If  the  release  and  removal  of  ACh  are  associated  with  the  primary 
alterations  of  the  nerve  membrane  during  the  passage  of  the  impulse, 
then  the  primary  source  of  the  chemical  energy  released  during  the  re- 
covery process  should  be  used  for  the  resynthesis  of  ACh. 

The  most  readily  available  source  of  energy  in  living  cells  is  that 
released  by  energy-rich  phosphate  bonds.  Phosphocreatine,  the  main 
"storehouse"  of  energy-rich  phosphate  bonds  in  muscle,  is  also  present 
in  nerves.  The  electric  organ  offers  a  suitable  material  for  investigat- 
ing the  chemical  reactions  which  supply  the  energ}^  for  the  action  po- 
tential. Both  electric  and  chemical  energy  released  are  within  the 
range  of  measurement,  whereas,  in  ordinary  nerves,  such  an  analysis 
is  difficult. 

Measurements  carried  out  on  the  electric  organ  of  Electrophorus 
electricus  have  revealed  that  the  chemical  energy  released  by  the  break- 
down of  phosphocreatine  is  adequate  to  account  for  the  electric  energy 
released  by  the  action  potential.  The  electric  energy  released  exter- 
nally per  gram  and  impulse,  in  large  eels,  was  found  to  be  4  micro- 
calories.  This  is  the  maximum  external  energy  which  may  be  obtained, 
under  the  condition  that  the  external  resistance  is  approximately  equal 
to  the  internal.  The  total  electric  energy  is  about  6  times  as  high  as 
the  external,  or  about  25  micro-calories.  These  data  were  obtained 
on  eels  of  170  to  180  cm.  length.     In  medium-sized  eels  of  90  to  120 


408  ANNALS  NEW  YORK  ACADEMY  OF  SCIENCES 

cm.  length,  the  total  electric  energy  released  per  gram  and  impulse  was 
found  to  be  47  micro-calories,  on  the  average.  There  are  some  as- 
sumptions, on  which  these  figures  are  based,  which  will  be  discussed  by 
Drs.  Cox,  Coates,  and  Brown.  If  we  consider  all  probable  assump- 
tions, these  figures  may  possibly  be  revised  downward  by  15  per  cent 
or  upward  up  to  100  per  cent. 

Tested  under  the  same  conditions,  the  energy  released  by  the  break- 
down of  phosphocreatine  was  found  to  be  32  micro-calories  per  gram 
and  impulse,  in  the  large  eels  (average  of  15  experiments).  In  the 
medium-sized  eels,  the  energy  released  by  phosphocreatine  was  about 
51  micro-calories  (average  of  15  experiments).  The  lactic  acid  forma- 
tion released  about  17  /xcal.,  in  the  large,  and  53  /xcal.  in  the  medium- 
sized,  eels  per  gram  and  impulse.  The  energy  of  the  lactic  acid  is  prob- 
ably used  to  rephosphorylate  creatine,  just  as  in  muscle  where  the 
phosphopyruvic  acid  transfers  its  phosphate  via  adenosine  triphosphate 
to  creatine  ("Parnas  reaction").  The  sum  of  the  two  reactions  may, 
therefore,  be  used  as  indication  for  the  energy  released  by  phosphate 
bonds.  It  amounts  to  49  /xcal.  in  the  large,  and  104  ;u,cal.  in  the 
medium-sized,  eels.  The  figures  are  consistent  with  the  conclusion 
that  energy-rich  phosphate  bonds  are  adequate  to  account  for  the  en- 
ergy of  the  action  potential. 

It  appeared  crucial  to  test  whether  or  not  energy-rich  phosphate 
bonds  are  really  the  energy  source  of  ACh  formation.  If  this  be  the 
case,  it  would  show  that  the  energy  of  the  primary  recovery  process  is 
really  used  for  the  resynthesis  of  the  compound  which,  by  its  release, 
supposedly  initiates  the  nerve  impulse.  It  would,  therefore,  at  the 
same  time,  constitute  a  new  support  for  the  assumption  that  the  primary 
"excitatory  disturbance"  which  produces  a  propagated  impulse  may, 
indeed,  be  the  release  of  the  ester. 

In  confirmation  of  this  assumption,  a  new  enzyme,  choline  acetylase, 
could  be  extracted  from  brain  in  cell  free  solution,  which,  under  strictly 
anaerobic  conditions,  in  presence  of  adenosine  triphosphate,  forms 
ACh."-3° 

The  enzyme  has  been  extracted  from  homogenized  brain.  From  one 
gram  of  fresh  rat  or  guinea  pig  brain,  enzyme  solutions  were  obtained 
which  form  150-200  /xg.  of  ACh  per  hour.  More  recently,  up  to  250 
;ag./g./hr.  were  obtained. 

Presence  of  eserine  and  fluoride  is  necessary  to  inhibit  the  action  of 
cholinesterase  and  adenosine  triphosphate,  respectively.  Inhibition  of 
the  latter  enzyme  is  necessary,  since,  otherwise,  the  breakdown  of 
adenosine   triphosphate   occurs  too   rapidly.     Fluoride   inhibits  this 


NACHMANSOHN:  CHEMICAL  MECHANISM  IN  NERVES        409 

breakdown,  but  it  does  not  interfere  with  the  transfer  of  energy-rich 
phosphate  bonds,  as  has  been  shown  by  Ochoa.^^ 

The  enzyme  has  also  been  extracted  from  powder  of  acetone  dried 
brain.2^'^°  Extracts  prepared  from  one  gram  of  powder  form  1.0-2.0 
mgs.  of  ACh  per  hour.  Since  acetone  inactivates  chohnesterase,  this 
enzyme  is  largely,  or  sometimes  completely,  inactivated  in  the  extracts 
prepared  from  powder  of  acetone  dried  brain,  so  that  addition  of  eserine 
may  have  either  a  small  effect  or  practically  none  on  the  formation  of 
ACh.  Adenosine  triphosphate  is  also  removed  in  extracts  from  acetone 
dried  brain.  No  addition  of  fluoride  is,  therefore,  required.  For  in- 
stance: In  one  experiment,  820  ^g.  of  ACh  were  formed  per  gram  and 
hour,  with  no  eserine  780  ^g.,  and  without  fluoride  810  ftg.  It  has, 
thus,  been  demonstrated  that  the  enzyme  mechanism  responsible  for 
the  formation  of  the  ester  is  not  identical  with  the  hydrolyzing  enzyme. 

The  enzyme  requires  the  presence  of  potassium  in  high  concentration, 
close  to  that  found  in  brain.  It  contains  active  sulfhydryl  groups 
which  are  readily  inactivated  by  monoiodoacetic  acid  or  copper  in  low 
concentration.  The  —  SH  groups  are  easily  oxidized  by  air.  On  di- 
alysis, the  enzyme  rapidly  loses  its  activity.  Addition  of  potassium  ion 
and  1  ( + )  glutamic  acid  or  cysteine  reactivates  partly.  1  ( + )  alanine, 
also,  has  some  effect;  other  amino  acids  have  either  a  weak  effect  or 
none.  Citric  acid  has  an  effect  nearly  as  strong  as  glutamic  acid, 
whereas  dicarboxylic  acids  have  practically  no  effect.^^-  ^° 

The  longer  the  dialysis  is  carried  on,  the  weaker  is  the  reactivation 
by  the  compounds  mentioned.  The  experiments  suggest  that  choline 
acetylase  requires,  a  coenzyme  for  its  activity.  The  coenzyme  has 
now  been  found.  In  contrast  to  the  enzyme  which  occurs  only  in 
nerve  tissue,  the  coenzyme  has  been  extracted  from  brain,  liver,  heart, 
and  skeletal  muscle  (Nachmansohn  and  Berman^^).  The  coenzyme 
has  been  purified  to  a  certain  degree  by  treatment  with  barium  salt, 
which  precipitates  the  coenzyme.  The  purification,  however,  is  still  in 
progress.  The  coenzyme  not  only  reactivates  the  dialyzed  enzyme, 
but  increases  considerably  the  undialyzed  enzyme  preparations. 
Marked  activation  has  been  obtained  in  this  way,  especially  in  extracts 
from  lobster  nerve,  rabbit's  optic  nerve,  ajid  electric  tissue.  Of  special 
interest  is  the  evidence  for  the  presence  of  choline  acetylase  in  the 
optic  nerve.  The  possibility  of  a  role  of  ACh  in  sensory  nerves  has 
been  a  matter  of  controversy  for  many  years,  since  the  ester  was  not 
found  in  such  nerves,  whereas  chohnesterase  is  present  in  concentra- 
tions in  an  order  of  magnitude  similar  to  that  in  motor  nerves.  The 
presence  of  choline  acetylase  in  the  optic  nerve  is  further  support  for 


410  ANNALS  NEW  YORK  ACADEMY  OF  SCIENCES 

the  assumption  that  ACh  may  have  the  same  function  there  as  in 
other  nerves. 

The  oxidation  products  of  amino  acids,  i.e.,  a-keto  acids,  have  a 
strong  inhibitory  effect  on  the  formation  of  ACh,  when  present  in  con- 
centrations of  10-^  to  10"*  M.  So  far,  pyruvic,  phenylpyruvic,  oxy- 
phenyl  pyruvic  acid,  and  a-keto  glutaric  acid  have  been  tested.^^'  •'*° 

ACh  formation  has  also  been  studied  in  extracts  prepared  from 
peripheral  nerve  fibers,  in  order  to  determine  whether  or  not  choline 
acetylase  is  present  in  the  peripheral  fibers,  as  well  as  in  brain. •■'^'  ^* 
This  should  be  the  case,  if  the  new  concept  of  the  role  of  the  ester  in  the 
axon  is  correct.  It  has  been  found  that  choline  acetylase  may  be  ex- 
tracted from  peripheral  nerve  fibers,  as  well  as  from  brain.  The  rate 
of  formation  of  ACh  in  extracts  prepared  from  the  sciatic  nerve  of  the 
rabbit  was  found  to  be  70  to  90  /xg.  per  gram  and  hour.  The  sciatic 
contains  a  large  amount  of  inactive  tissue  (connective  tissue,  fat,  and 
myelin).  On  the  assumption  that  this  tissue  forms  about  two-thirds 
of  the  total  weight,  which  is  a  conservative  estimate,  the  amount  of 
ACh  which  can  be  formed  in  the  axon  of  the  rabbit  sciatic  may,  thus, 
be  about  250  fxg.  per  gram  per  hour,  and  is  probably  higher. 

It  appeared  of  special  interest  to  determine  the  activity  of  choline 
acetylase  during  degeneration,  and  to  test  how  this  metabolism  is  re- 
lated to  the  nerve  function,  i.e.,  to  conductivity.  Conduction  is  still 
maintained  two  days  after  section,  whereas,  after  three  days,  it  has 
disappeared.  If  the  release  of  ACh  is  responsible  for  conductivity, 
formation  of  ACh  should  be  possible  at  a  rate  not  too  far  below  normal, 
as  long  as  the  nerve  is  capable  of  conducting. 

Forty-eight  hours  after  the  section  of  the  sciatic,  choline  acetylase 
activity  has  decreased  only  about  20  to  25  per  cent.  After  seventy- 
two  hours,  when  conductivity  has  disappeared,  the  decrease  is  marked, 
but  still  about  one-third  of  the  enzyme  is  present.  The  results  are 
consistent  with  the  assumption  that  enzyme  mechanism  is  required  for 
conduction. 

C.     Nerve  Action  Potential  and  Inhibition  of  Cholinesterase 

In  a  third  line  of  investigation,  cholinesterase  activity  and  nerve 
action  potential  could  be  directly  correlated  in  experiments  on  the 
peripheral  axon.  One  of  the  essential  facts  in  support  of  the  theory 
of  "neuro-humoral"  or  "synaptic"  transmission  was  the  observation 
that  ACh,  when  applied  to  synaptic  regions,  may  have  a  stimulating  ac- 
tion. No  action  has  yet  been  obtained  with  the  ester,  when  applied 
to  the  axon.     Lorente  de  N6^^  kept  bullfrogs'  sciatic  nerve  in  a  two 


NACHMANSOHN:  CHEMICAL  MECHANISM  IN  NERVES        411 

gram  per  cent  solution  of  ACh  for  many  hours,  and  did  not  find  any 
effect  on  conductivity.  He  considers  his  failure  to  obtain  an  effect  on 
the  axon  by  ACh  as  proof  against  the  new  concept  of  the  role  of  ACh 
in  the  mechanism  of  nerve  activity.  ACh  is  a  quaternary  ammonium 
salt.  Such  compounds  are  completely  ionized  and  usually  lipoid 
insoluble.  Generally,  they  do  not  penetrate  the  lipoid  membrane. 
Therefore,  these  compounds  can  be  expected  to  have  no  effect  on  the 
axon,  since  axons  are  always  surrounded  by  a  lipoid  membrane,  even 
though  it  may  be  rather  thin. 


1 


i 


V 


A 


'iT 


Figure  5.     Effect  of  eserine  on  single  fiber  action  potential   (giant  axon  of  squid). 

Left:  eyerine  0.002M,  records  (from  top  to  bottom)  at  0',  10',  25'  (conduction  abolished),  35'; 
sea  water  at  26'.     Conduction  distance  less  for  last  record,  too  short  to  demonstrate  latency  effect. 

Right:  eserine  O.OIM,  records  at  0',  15',  fiber  then  rinsed,  and  axoplasm  analyzed  chemically. 
Upper  time  scale  applies  to  this  experiment,  lower  to  column  at  left,  both  1000  c.p.s. 

The  problem  has  been  approached  in  a  different  way:  If  ACh  is  the 
depolarizing  agent  and  if  the  function  of  cholinesterase  is  to  remove 
the  active  ester,  so  that  polarization  again  becomes  possible  after  the 
passage  of  the  impulse,  then  inhibition  of  the  enzyme  should  alter,  and, 
in  sufficiently  high  concentration,  abolish,  the  nerve  action  potential. ^"^ 

Eserine  is  known  to  be  a  strong  inhibitor  of  cholinesterase.  This 
compound  is  a  tertiary  amine  and  may,  therefore,  if  undissociated, 
penetrate  the  lipoid  membrane.     Experiments  carried  out  on  the  giant 


412  ANNALS  NEW  YORK  ACADEMY  OF  SCIENCES 

axon  and  on  the  fin  nerve  of  squid  have  shown  that  eserine  alters,  and, 
in  higher  concentrations,  abohshes,  the  nerve  action  potential.  Within 
a  few  minutes  in  eserine,  amplitude,  length,  and  duration  of  the  action 
potential  recorded  with  the  cathode  ray  oscillograph  are  markedly 
changed,  and  in  20  to  25  minutes,  the  conductivity  has  been  abolished 
(figure  5) .  When  the  nerves  are  put  back  into  sea  water,  they 
c^uickly  recover,  and  conductivity  reappears.  The  reversibility  of  the 
effect  is  consistent  with  the  fact  that  the  inhibition  of  cholinesterase 
is  easily  reversible  in  vitro. 

Strychnine,  another  inhibitor  of  cholinesterase,  was  also  found  to 
alter,  and,  in  higher  concentrations,  to  abolish,  the  nerve  action  poten- 
tial reversibly. 


-^1 


'/- 


J 


'^r 


Figure   6.    Effect  of   prostigmine  on  single  fiber  action  potential   (giant   axon).     Records  before 
and  after  45'  in  O.OIM. 

Thus,  a  new  relationship  has  been  established  between  enzyme  activ- 
ity and  nerve  action  potential,  in  this  case  using  the  peripheral  axon. 

Prostigmine  has,  in  vitro,  the  same  effect  as  eserine,  but  it  has  no 
effect  on  the  nerve  action  potential  (figures  6  and  7).  Prostigmine 
is  like  ACh,  a  quaternary  ammonium  salt,  and  it  cannot  penetrate  the 
lipoid  membrane.  This  has  been  demonstrated  by  the  following  ex- 
periment. The  axoplasm  of  the  nerves  kept  in  eserine  was  extruded, 
and  the  presence  of  the  compound  was  tested  by  the  inhibitory  effect 
on  a  purified  cholinesterase  solution.  Even  in  thousand-fold  dilution, 
the  axoplasm  from  a  portion  of  a  single  axon  showed,  by  the  inhibition 


NACHMANSOHN:  CHEMICAL  MECHANISM  IN  NERVES       413 

of  esterase,  easily  detectable  quantities  of  eserine.  The  axoplasm  of 
nerves  kept  in  prostigmine  had  no  inhibitory  effect  on  cholinesterase, 
even  when  undiluted. 

Prostigmine,  like  ACh,  has  3  methyl  groups  attached  to  the  nitrogen. 
Drs.  Bronk  and  Acheson  have  offered  evidence  that  tetraethylammo- 
nium  chloride  acts  on  medullated  nerve  and,  therefore,  presumably  en- 
ters it.     This  compound  is  also  a  quaternary  ammonium  salt  and  com- 


MiNo 


MS. 


Figure  7.    Effect  of  prostigmine  on  fin  nerve. 
O.OIM.     Traced  from  enlarged   photographs. 


Records  before  and  after  83',  205',  and  370'  in 


pletely  dissociated.  Although  it  is  true  that  ionized  compounds  are 
not  readily  soluble  in  lipoids,  the  properties  which  decide  lipoid  solu- 
bility are  far  from  well  established.  Frequently,  when,  in  a  com- 
pound, the  ratio  of  C  over  N  is  increased,  it  becomes  more  lipoid 
soluble.  In  tetraethylammonium  chloride,  there  is  four  times  as 
much  carbon  as  in  tetramethylammonium  chloride.     The  change  from 


414  ANNALS  NEW  YORK  ACADEMY  OF  SCIENCES 

methyl  to  ethyl  affects  profoundly  the  physico-chemical  properties 
of  a  molecule.  The  difference  between  methyl  and  ethyl  alcohol  is 
well  known,  and  need  not  be  discussed  here. 

Nearly  half  a  century  ago,  Michaelis  showed  that,  if,  in  a  certain  dye, 
the  ethyl  groups  were  substituted  by  methyl  groups,  no  staining  in- 
side the  living  cell  could  be  obtained. ^^  Since  these  groups  are  not 
part  of  the  molecule  which  has  the  staining  properties,  the  loss  of 
staining  power  may  be  due  to  the  impossibility  of  penetrating  the  cell, 
due  to  the  substitution  performed.  It  appears  not  surprising  that  a 
compound  with  4  ethyl  groups  becomes  lipoid  soluble,  in  spite  of  nearly 
complete  dissociation. 

The  inability  to  penetrate  the  lipoid  membrane  may  explain  why 
ACh  and  prostigmine,  applied  externally,  act  only  on  nerve  endings 
which  do  not  have  a  myelin  sheath,  but  are  inactive  when  applied  to 
the  axon.  Only  in  electric  tissue  may  the  power  of  ACh  to  produce 
an  action  potential  be  demonstrated.  Injection  of  ACh  leads  to 
changes  in  potential  of  the  same  direction  as  those  observed  during 
the  discharge.^^  Electric  tissue,  however,  is  an  accumulation  of  end- 
plates  which,  in  contrast  to  the  axons,  are  not  protected  by  myeline 
and,  therefore,  do  react.  This  may  also  be  the  explanation  for  the 
famous  observation  of  Claude  Bernard  on  the  effect  of  curare,  since, 
according  to  recent  observations,  the  active  principle  of  curare  is  a 
quaternary  ammonium  salt.^^'  *° 

The  peculiar  ability  of  the  synapse  to  react  to  injected  ACh  can  no 
longer  be  referred  to  a  difference  in  the  fundamental  physico-chemical 
process  underlying  the  propagation  of  the  nerve  impulse,  but  to  the 
difference  in  histological  structure. 

Effect  of  Di-Isopropyl  Fluorophosphate  (DFP)* 

Recently,  a  new  inhibitor  of  cholinesterase,  di-isopropyl  fluoro- 
phosphate (DFP),  became  known,  which  can  inhibit  cholinesterase 
irreversibly.  Tested  on  the  fin  nerve  of  squid,  the  compound  has  the 
same  effect  on  the  action  potential  as  was  observed  with  eserine,  and 
at  about  the  same  concentration.^^  When  the  nerve  is  kept  in  a  solu- 
tion of  2  mgs.  of  DFP  per  cc,  the  action  potential  is  completely  abol- 
ished in  about  30  minutes.  When  the  nerve  is  put  back  into  sea  water, 
the  action  potential  comes  back  (figure  8) .  These  experiments  sug- 
gested that,  for  relatively  short  periods  and  at  low  temperature,  around 
20°  C,  the  inactivation  of  cholinesterase  by  DFP  may  be  partly  re- 


*  Most  of  the  observations  reported  in  this  paragraph  were  carried  out  after  the  conference,  but, 
since  the  effect  of  fluorophosphate  on  the  action  potential  and  its  mechanism  played  an  important 
role  then,  it  appeared  desirable  to  include  these  data  in  this  paper. 


NACHMANSOHN:  CHEMICAL  MECHANISM  IN  NERVES        415 

versible.  The  cholinesterase  activity  in  squid  nerves,  under  the  ex- 
perimental conditions  used,  could  not  be  determined,  since  the  season 
was  too  advanced,  and  no  squids  were  available.  Experiments  were 
therefore  carried  out  with  the  abdominal  nerve  cord  of  the  lob- 
ster. This  nerve  preparation  has  a  high  cholinesterase  activity  and 
relatively  satisfactory  action  potentials.  The  potentials  recorded  were 
exclusively  those  of  the  giant  axons  of  the  cord.  The  transmission 
across  the  synapses  in  the  ganglia  does  not,  therefore,  enter  into  the 
picture. 


Ftgurr  8.      Effect  of  DFP  on  thp  action  potentinl  of  the  fin  nerve  of  sai"H. 
DFP  0.013M.    First  two  records  (from  top  to  bottom):  before,  and  after,  35'  in  DFP.    The  last 
two  records :  recovery  after  60'  and  215'  in  sea  water. 

When  the  nerve  is  immersed  in  a  solution  of  2  mgs.  of  DFP  per  cc, 
the  action  potential  disappears  within  about  30  to  40  minutes,  as  in 
the  case  of  squid  nerve.  If  the  nerve  preparation  is  put  back  into  sea 
water,  the  action  potential  reappears  after  some  time.  Nerves  kept  in 
DFP  for  additional  periods  after  the  abolition  of  the  action  potential 
show  less  complete  recovery.  Exposure  of  the  nerve  to  DFP,  for  90 
minutes  after  the  disappearance  of  the  action  potential,  abolishes  the 
response  irreversibly. 

Determinations  of  cholinesterase  in  these  nerves  reveal  a  striking 
parallelism  between  the  recovery  of  the  action  potential  and  the  re- 
appearance of  cholinesterase  (figure  9).    The  less  complete  the  re- 


416 


^A^A^^L-S  NEW  YORK  ACADEMY  OF  SCIENCES 


h 


-^^Iw*  ->  "'\L^  V*iv^  -r^Kr 


'■'  r 


-  v> 


'IJ^ 


Figure  9.  Reversibility  of  action  potential  and  reappearance  of  cholinesterase  in  nerves  ex- 
posed for  varying  periods  of  time  to  DFP,  0.013M. 

The  nerve  whose  action  potentials  are  shown  in  Column  1  was  transferred  to  sea  water  imme- 
diately after  the  action  potential  was  abolished,  and  washed  for  one  hour.  The  nerves  of  Column 
2  to  4  were  kept  in  DFP  for  30',  60',  and  90',  after  the  action  potential  had  disappeared,  and  then 
washed  in  sea  water.  The  top  line  of  each  column  shows  the  action  potential  in  the  untreated 
nerves.  The  second  line  shows  the  abolition  of  the  response  by  DFP.  The  third  line  shows  the 
degree  of  recovery  after  washing  the  nerve.  The  reappearance  of  cholinesterase  activity  is  shown 
in  the  vertical  bars  of  the  fourth  line.  The  CO2  output  is  233,  129,  88.5,  and  50  cmm.  per  100 
mgs.  per  hour. 

covery  of  the  action  potential,  the  smaller  is  the  amount  of  cholin- 
esterase activity.  Even  after  complete  and  irreversible  abolition  of 
the  action  potential,  a  small  amount  of  enzyme  activity  may  still  be 
detected.  The  experiments  indicate  that  cholinesterase  inhibition  by 
DFP  of  cold-blooded  animals  is  partly  reversible,  for  a  certain  period 
of  time. 

This  has  been  confirmed  by  observations  on  in  vitro  inhibition  of 
cholinesterase  solution.  DFP  was  added,  in  two  different  concentra- 
tions, 0.1  and  0.5  [xg.  per  cc,  to  cholinesterase  solution  prepared  from 
electric  tissue.     At  the  low  concentration  of  DFP,  the  enzyme  solution 


NACHMANSOHN:  CHEMICAL  MECHANISM  IN  NERVES        417 

liberated  520  cmm.  CO2,  instead  of  790  cmm.  CO2  per  hour  without 
DFP.  The  activity  decreased  slo"wly,  over  a  period  of  hours.  In 
the  solution  exposed  to  the  stronger  concentration  of  DFP,  the  activity 
was  only  25  per  cent  and  was  nearly  completely  abolished  after  30 
minutes.  If,  after  varying  periods  of  exposure  of  the  enzyme  solution 
to  DFP  in  greater  concentration,  the  solution  was  diluted,  part  of  the 
activity  could  be  retained  for  a  period  of  two  to  three  hours  (figure 
10) .  These  experiments  give  additional  evidence  that  the  irreversible 
inhibition  of  cholinesterase  by  DFP  is  a  slow  process  at  low  tem- 
perature. 


MINUTES 

Figure    10.    Reversibility    of    cholinesterase    inhibition    bv    DFP    in    vitro,    tested    by    dilution 
effect,  t  =  9°  C. 

The  cholinesterase  solutidn  used  liberates  790  cmm.  CO2  per  hour. 
+  —  +   Activity  found  in  presence  of  0.1  /ig.  of  DFP  per  cc. 
O  — ^  O    Activity  found  in  presence  of  0.5  fig.  of  DFP  per  cc. 
•  —  •    Activity  found  after  exposure  to  0.5  /ig.  of  DFP  per  cc,  for  varj'ing  periods  of  time, 

and  subsequent  dilution  to  0.1  ng.   per  cc.      The  part  with  the  dotted  lines  indicates 

the  reversibility  as  a  function  of  time. 

Dr.  Oilman  presented  observations  on  bullfrogs,  in  which  it  was 
found  that,  following  injection  of  DFP,  the  action  potential  of  the 
sciatic  nerve  may  persist  in  the  apparent  absence  of  cholinesterase. 
The  bullfrog  sciatic  nerve  contains  extremely  small  amounts  of  cholin- 
esterase. 100  mgs.  of  nerve  (wet  weight)  liberate  40-50  cmm.  CO2 
per  hour.  Observations  on  lobster  nerve  indicate  that  the  enzyme  is 
present  in  about  five  times  excess,  since  about  80  per  cent  may  be  re- 
moved while  the  action  potential  is  unaffected.  Even  if,  in  the  bull- 
frog sciatic  nerve,  the  excess  of  enzyme  is  smaller  when  part  of  the 


418  ANNALS  NEW  YORK  ACADEMY  OF  SCIENCES 

activity  disappears,  the  measurement  of  the  CO2  liberation  falls  into 
a  range  where  precise  evaluation  becomes  difficult.  Moreover,  in  such 
a  preparation,  the  retention  of  CO2  by  the  protein  becomes  an  impor- 
tant factor.  Finally,  even  in  the  thin  lipoid  membrane  of  the  lobster 
nerve  after  prolonged  washing,  sufficient  excess  of  DFP  is  retained 
to  inhibit  20-40  per  cent  of  the  remaining  esterase  activity.  At 
least  this  amount,  if  not  more,  may  be  retained  in  the  relatively  greater 
amount  of  myelin  and  fat  in  the  bullfrog  sciatic  nerve.  When  this 
nerve  is  then  ground,  the  retained  DFP  may  come  in  contact  with  the 
cholinesterase  and  destroy  a  considerable  fraction  of  the  enzyme  still 
present  in  the  intact  nerve. 

DISCUSSION 

It  may  be  of  interest  to  discuss  the  neuro-humoral  theory  in  the  light 
of  recent  developments,  and  to  analyze  the  two  basic  experiments  which 
form  the  main  support  for  the  hypothesis  that  the  ester  is  actually 
liberated  at  the  nerve  ending  and,  having  crossed  the  synapse  or  motor 
end-plates,  acts  directly  on  the  second  neuron  or  on  the  muscle  fiber. 
The  two  observations  are:  (1)  The  stimulating  action  of  ACh  when 
applied  to  synaptic  regions;  (2)  the  appearance  of  the  ester  in  the 
perfusion  fluid,  following  nerve  stimulation.  It  has  just  been  explained 
why  the  effect  of  ACh  applied  externally  is  limited  to  the  nerve  ending. 
In  any  case,  a  stimulating  effect  is  not  necessarily  a  physiological  effect, 
but  may  well  be  a  pharmacological  one.  The  same  action  may,  indeed, 
be  produced  by  other  compounds.  The  observation  of  Otto  Loewi  that 
a  compound  appears  in  the  perfusion  fluid,  following  nerve  stimulation, 
was  important  because  it  suggested  that  the  compound  may  be  con- 
nected with  nerve  activity.  The  importance  of  this  observation  need 
not  be  minimized  because  a  quarter  of  a  century  later  the  original  in- 
terpretation has  to  be  changed.  In  fact,  by  the  new  development,  the 
role  of  ACh  became  more  general  and  more  important  than  could  orig- 
inally have  been  anticipated.  The  appearance  of  a  compound  in  the  per- 
fusion fluid,  however,  is  not  sufficient  evidence  for  concluding  that  the 
compound  acts  outside  the  cell.  Many  compounds  of  intermediate 
cell  metabolism  may  appear  outside  the  cell.  This  is  due  to  the  fact 
that  all  enzymatic  reactions  follow  a  logarithmic  curve.  Therefore, 
if  even  the  greatest  part  of  a  compound  is  rapidly  metabolized  by  the 
intracellular  enzymes,  a  small  fraction  may  persist  long  enough  to 
escape  enzymatic  action  and  leak  out  from  the  cell.  This,  apparently, 
may  happen  also  to  ACh,  in  spite  of  the  high  concentration  of  cholin- 


NACHMANSOHN:  CHEMICAL  MECHANISM  IN  NERVES        419 

esterase  inside  the  cell,  particularly  when  some  kind  of  damage  of  the 
surface  membrane  is  produced,  as  may  be  expected  in  the  case  of  pro- 
longed perfusion  or  in  other  unphysiological  conditions  affecting  either 
the  membrane  permeability  or  the  cholinesterase  activity. 

In  order  to  verify  the  assumption  that  the  amount  of  ACh  actually 
released  from  the  nerve  ending  is  sufficiently  high  to  produce  a  stim- 
ulating effect  on  the  second  unit,  Dale  and  his  associates  attempted 
two  sets  of  experiments.  They  determined  the  minimum  required  to 
produce  a  stimulus  and  compared  it  to  the  amounts  released.  How- 
ever, in  both  cases  tested,  a  puzzling  discrepancy  was  found:  In  the 
case  of  the  superior  cervical  ganglion,  only  1/40,000  of  the  amount  of 
ACh  necessary  to  produce  a  single  response  appeared  in  the  perfusion 
fluid  per  impulse.  In  the  case  of  the  muscle,  only  1/100,000  of  the 
amount  of  ACh  necessary  to  produce  a  single  twitch  was  collected. 
This  difference  is  so  considerable  that  the  observations  cannot  be  con- 
sidered as  evidence  for  the  idea  that  ACh  is  the  direct  transmitter  of 
the  impulse,  especially  in  view  of  all  the  other  obstacles. 

The  situation  is  further  complicated  by  the  fact  that  these  infinitely 
small  amounts  of  ACh  can  only  be  found  in  presence  of  eserine  which 
should  inhibit  their  destruction.  The  enzyme  located  at  the  neuro- 
nal surface  forms  a  barrier  for  the  crossing  of  the  ester.  Even 
without  regarding  the  existing  discrepancy,  it  is  very  difficult  to  believe 
that,  under  physiological  conditions,  that  is,  in  the  absence  of  eserine, 
the  small  amounts  of  ACh  released  can  cross  the  barrier  and  still  arrive 
in  sufficient  concentrations  for  producing  a  response.  The  small 
amounts  found  under  these  conditions  are  easily  explained  if  we  as- 
sume that  ACh  is  released  inside  the  cell,  and  that  the  amounts  which 
appear  in  the  perfusion  fluid  are  those  which  have  escaped  hydrolysis 
and  have  been  preserved,  due  to  the  presence  of  eserine. 

Another  question  on  which  some  comments  may  be  useful,  is  that  of 
the  difference  between  the  rates  of  ACh  formation  and  hydrolysis. 
There  are  two  instances  in  which  these  two  rates  may  be  compared 
on  the  basis  of  experimentally  established  data:  The  guinea  pig  brain 
and  the  rabbit  sciatic  nerve.  In  the  first  case,  about  200  to  250  ;u,g. 
of  ACh  may  be  formed,  whereas  about  70  mgs.  may  be  split  per  gram 
per  hour.  The  rate  of  cholinesterase  activity  is,  thus,  about  300  to 
350  times  higher  than  that  of  choline  acetylase.  In  rabbit  sciatic 
nerve,  the  figures  are  about  100  /xg.  per  gram  per  hour  and  15-20  mgs. 
per  gram  per  hour,  i.e.,  the  rate  of  hydrolysis  is  about  150-200  times 


420  ANNALS  NEW  YORK  ACADEMY  OF  SCIENCES 

as  high  as  that  of  synthesis.  It  is  doubtful  whether  these  figures  in- 
dicate the  real  difference  of  the  possible  rates  of  the  two  enzymes. 
Cholinesterase  is  an  extremely  stable  enzyme.  Its  activity  is  deter- 
mined in  a  well  ground  and  homogenized  suspension  of  the  tissue.  It 
appears  probable  that  the  maximal  possible  activity  is  actually  meas- 
ured in  vitro.  This  is  almost  certainly  not  the  case  with  choline 
acetylase.  The  enzyme  is  an  extremely  labile  and  a  rather  complex 
system  which  has  to  be  extracted  from  the  tissue.  During  the  prepara- 
tion, part  of  the  activity  may  have  been  lost.  We  do  not  know 
whether  the  conditions  used  at  present  are  optimal  or  even  close  to 
optimal.  Although  the  enzyme  was  discovered  three  years  ago,  the 
rates  of  formation  obtained  are  still  continuously  increasing,  since  more 
and  more  factors  are  becoming  known  which  activate  the  enzyme 
(Nachmansohn  and  Berman,  unpublished  experiments).  In  such  a 
case,  it  is  possible  and,  in  fact,  probable,  that  the  activity  in  vivo  may 
be  considerably  higher  than  that  observed  in  the  solution.  A  sharp 
distinction  has,  moreover,  to  be  made  between  the  potential  and  the 
actual  rate.  Rates  of  enzymes  measured  in  vitro  are  potential  rates. 
The  actual  rates  in  the  living  cell  may  be  entirely  different.  Many 
enzymes  are  present  in  excess  in  the  cell.  An  excess  of  3  to  5  times 
above  the  actual  requirement  is  nothing  unusual.  A  5-fold  excess  of 
cholinesterase  above  that  necessary  for  function  has  been  recently 
observed  in  the  case  of  lobster  nerve  (Bullock  et  aL*^) .  Other  enzymes 
are  in  much  greater  excess,  whereas,  in  some  cases,  the  excess  activity 
appears  to  be  relatively  small.  Nothing  is  known,  at  present,  as  to 
whether  or  not  choline  acetylase  is  present  in  excess.  Even  if  this 
is  the  case,  it  may  be  much  smaller  than  that  of  cholinesterase. 

For  an  understanding  of  the  problem,  the  decisive  difference  which 
has  to  be  considered  is  not  the  difference  of  rates,  but  the  difference  of 
function.  There  is  a  fundamental  difference  between  the  function  of 
cholinesterase  and  that  of  choline  acetylase.  If  the  release  of  ACh 
is  an  essential  event  in  the  alterations  of  the  membrane  during  the 
passage  of  the  impulse,  then  the  active  ester  has  to  be  destroyed  within 
a  millisecond  or  less,  so  that  the  resting  condition  may  be  restored. 
Therefore,  the  enzyme  which  removes  the  active  ester,  cholinesterase, 
has  to  be  very  active,  but  only  during  this  brief  period,  and  may  then 
be  inactive  until  the  passage  of  the  next  impulse.  The  formation  of 
ACh,  on  the  other  hand,  need  not  be  such  a  rapid  process.  It  is  gen- 
erally assumed  that  the  active  ester  is  released  from  an  inactive  form. 
This  is  supported  by  the  fact  that  the  primary  energy  released  during 
recovery  is  used  for  the  synthesis  of  ACh,  thus  implying  that  the  syn- 


NACHMANSOHN:  CHEMICAL  MECHANISM  IN  NERVES        421 

thesis  is  a  slow  recovery  process.  It  is,  therefore,  not  difficult  to  as- 
sume that  the  period  required  for  the  formation  is  longer  than  that  for 
the  hydrolysis  of  the  same  amount,  according  to  the  kind  of  nerve, 
its  condition,  temperature,  and  so  on. 

In  the  initial  phase  of  nerve  stimulation,  the  preformed  ACh  would 
act  as  a  reserve  and  would  make  conductivity  independent  of  the  rate 
of  ACh  formation  for  a  considerable  length  of  time.  Even  the  few 
/ig.  of  ACh  found  per  gram  of  nerve  would  be  sufficient  to  make  possible 
the  passage  of  several  thousand  impulses.  The  actual  amount  of  pre- 
formed ACh  in  the  living  cell  may  be  higher  than  that  found  experi- 
mentally, since  it  is  possible  that,  during  the  destruction  of  the  cell,  a 
process  necessary  for  the  determination,  a  considerable  part  of  the 
preformed  ester  is  destroyed.  A  nerve  should,  therefore,  be  able  to 
respond  to  stimulation  for  a  considerable  length  of  time,  independent 
of  the  rate  of  ACh  formation.  Only  in  cases  of  prolonged  stimulation 
should  the  rate  of  formation  become  the  limiting  factor.  If  all  pre- 
formed ACh  has  been  exhausted,  and  stimuli  are  applied  to  mammalian 
nerve  every  five  milliseconds,  then  the  amount  synthesized  in  the 
intervals  between  stimuli  should  be  sufficient  for  producing  the 
necessary  alterations  in  the  membrane  when  released  by  a  stimulus,  and 
should  be  equivalent  to  the  amount  actually  destroyed,  during  the 
passage  of  the  impulse,  by  cholinesterase.  Since,  in  mammalian  nerve, 
the  duration  of  the  spike  is  only  0.5  millisecond  and  the  cholinesterase 
may  have  acted  only  during  part  of  this  period,  e.g.,  0.1  or  0.2  milli- 
seconds, a  difference  of  25  to  50  times  between  the  actual  rate  of  cholin- 
esterase and  that  of  choline  acetylase  activity  would  keep  the  nerve 
going  indefinitely,  if  this  were  the  only  factor  involved. 

In  summary,  considering  the  difference  between  the  rates  of  cholin- 
esterase and  choline  acetylase,  we  have  to  keep  in  mind:  (1)  that  there 
is  a  fundamental  difference  of  function;  (2)  that  the  cholinesterase 
activity  determined  is  probably  the  maximum  possible,  whereas  the 
choline  acetylase  activity  found  in  vitro  is  almost  certainly  below  the 
optimal  rate  in  vivo;  (3)  that  the  excess  of  cholinesterase  may  be 
greater  than  that  of  choline  acetylase.  In  view  of  this  situation,  the 
difference  between  the  rates  found  does  not  offer  any  difficulty  and, 
in  fact,  appears  close  to  that  which  one  would  expect  of  these  two 
enzymes  so  different  in  function  and  properties. 

As  to  the  criticism  of  Dr.  Gerard,  who  resolutely  rejects  the  con- 
cept presented,  some  of  his  main  objections  may  be  discussed  briefly. 
(1)  The  high  speed  required  for  any  chemical  reaction  associated  with 
the  transmission  of  the  nerve  impulse  has  been  considereii-ler-^^P^S 


422  ANNALS  NEW  YORK  ACADEMY  OF  SCIENCES 

time,  by  many  leading  physiologists  as  the  chief  difficulty  for  any 
chemical  theory.  It  is  gratifying  to  see  that  the  evidence  accumu- 
lated during  the  last  ten  years  for  the  high  rate  of  cholinesterase  activ- 
ity appears  to  be  so  impressive  that  Dr.  Gerard  now  sees  in  this  high 
speed  one  of  the  main  difficulties.  He  calculates,  for  example,  that 
the  ACh  preformed,  plus  that  synthesized,  could  not  possibly  supply 
the  ester  as  fast  as  cholinesterase  can  split  it. 

Such  an  objection  would  only  hold  if  the  whole  amount  of  cholin- 
esterase present  were  continuously  and  fully  active.  It  appears  likely, 
however,  that,  at  any  given  moment,  only  part  of  the  enzyme  acts  and 
only  for  extremely  brief  periods.  The  differences  found  between  the 
rates  of  formation  and  removal  of  ACh  appear,  as  pointed  out  before, 
to  be  well  within  the  expected  range. 

(2)  Still  more  puzzling  to  Dr.  Gerard  is  the  fact  that,  at  the  motor 
end-plate,  there  is  15,000  times  more  cholinesterase  than  in  the  sur- 
rounding muscle  fiber,  since  there  is  no  evidence  for  a  great  store  or 
synthesis  of  ACh  at  this  junction. 

The  difference  between  muscle  fiber  and  end-plate  is  interesting,  in 
view  of  the  specialized  localization.  It  is  comparable  to  the  distribution 
found  in  nerve  between  *surf ace  and  axoplasm,  which  is  infinite.  In 
absolute  amounts,  the  ACh  which  can  be  metabolized  per  impulse  per 
end-plate  is  0.000002  fx,g.  The  formation  of  this  amount  does  not  re- 
quire a  particularly  powerful  synthesizing  system  nor  an  intensive 
respiration.  The  energy  required  for  the  synthesis,  even  assuming  a 
high  frequency,  would  still  amount  to  less  than  one  per  cent  of  the 
oxidative  energy  measured,  a  deviation  which  is  far  below  the  meas- 
urable range. 

(3)  Dr.  Gerard  assumes  that  the  heat  production  by  the  ACh  re- 
leased would  amount  to  10  per  cent  of  the  total  heat,  whereas  the 
initial  heat  is  only  about  3  per  cent. 

The  frog's  sciatic  nerve  is  suitable  for  such  a  calculation,  since  here 
more  experimental  data  are  available  than  in  other  cases.  According 
to  von  Muralt,  0.0006  /xg.  of  ACh  is  released  per  gram  per  impulse.*^ 
This  would  amount  to  about  6  X  10"^  gram  calories,  which  is  0.6  per 
cent  of  the  total  or  20  per  cent  of  the  initial  heat. 

(4)  Finally,  many  other  agents  and  enzymes  are  present  in  neurons, 
like  adrenaline,  thiamin,  adenosine  triphosphate,  COo,  and  many  others. 
Dr.  Gerard  asks  how  we  can  reasonably  select  ACh  and  assign  to  it 
alone  an  essential  role  in  conduction  of  the  nervous  impulse.  Un- 
doubtedly, there  are  other  compounds  and  enzymes  playing  an  essential 
role  in  nerve  activity.     The  ACh  cycle  is  evidence  for  that.     But  none 


NACHMANSOHN:  CHEMICAL  MECHANISM  IN  NERVES        423 

of  these  compounds  shows  the  typical  features  of  the  ACh  system,  like 
the  high  speed,  the  exclusive  localization  in  the  surface,  the  parallelism 
with  voltage,  etc.    These  unique  features  of  the  ACh  system  make  it 
possible  to  associate  the  ester  more  closely  with  the  action  potential  !* 
than  all  other  agents  so  far  known. 

SUMMARY  AND  CONCLUSION 

In  view  of  the  complex  nature  of  biological  mechanisms,  one  or  two 
facts,  however  well  established  and  suggestive,  would  not  be  sufficient 
for  any  theory.  However,  if  a  great  number  of  facts  point  in  the  same 
direction,  then  they  support  each  other  and  potentiate  the  value  of  each 
of  them.  The  essential  facts  established  may  be  summarized:  (1) 
The  high  concentration  of  cholinesterase  in  nerve  tissue  makes  possible 
the  removal  of  ACh  at  a  speed  comparable  to  that  of  the  electric  mani- 
festations. (2)  Cholinesterase  is  localized  everywhere  at  the  neuronal 
surface  where  the  bioelectrical  phenomena  occur.  The  exclusive  local- 
ization in  the  surface  contrasts  strikingly  with  the  localization  of  other 
enzymes.  (3)  Cholinesterase  in  nervous  tissue  (and  in  muscle)  is 
distinctly  different  from  all  other  tissue  esterases  occurring  in  the  body. 
The  enzyme,  present  in  all  types  of  nerves  throughout  the  entire  animal 
kingdom,  shows  similar  properties.  (4)  A  direct  proportionality  be- 
tween voltage  and  cholinesterase  activity  has  been  established  in  the 
electric  organ  of  Electrophorus  electricus.  (5)  The  primary  energy 
source  of  recovery  after  the  passage  of  the  impulse,  namely,  the  energy- 
rich  phosphate  bonds  of  adenosine  triphosphate,  is  used  for  ACh  syn- 
thesis. (6)  The  formation  of  ACh  by  choline  acetylase  occurs  at  a 
high  rate  in  the  peripheral  fibers,  as  well  as  in  the  brain.  The  enzyme 
has,  so  far,  been  found  exclusively  in  nerve  tissue.  (7)  Anticholin- 
esterases alter,  and,  in  high  concentrations,  abolish,  the  nerve  action 
potential.  The  abolition  of  the  action  potential  is  reversible,  if  the 
inhibition  of  cholinesterase  is  reversible;  irreversible  inhibition  of 
cholinesterase  abolishes  the  nerve  action  potential  irreversibly. 

These  facts  considered  altogether  make  it  highly  probable  that  the 
release  and  removal  of  ACh  is  an  intracellular  event,  directly  associated 
with  the  nerve  action  potential. 

The  precise  function  of  the  ester  is  still  a  matter  of  interpretation. 
On  the  basis  of  the  physical  and  chemical  data  available,  one  possible 
interpretation  appears  to  be  that  the  ester  plays  an  essential  role  in 
the  breakdown  of  the  membrane  resistance,  occurring  during  the  pass- 
age of  the  impulse.     New  facts  may  change  the  situation.     A  number 


424  ANNALS  NEW  YORK  ACADEMY  OF  SCIENCES 

of  questions  still  have  to  be  answered  before  a  satisfactory  picture  of 
the  chemical  mechanisms  of  nervous  action  can  be  obtained. 

BIBLIOGRAPHY 

1.  Erlanger,  J.,  &  H.  S.  Gasser 

1937.     Electrical  Signs  of  Nervous  Activity.     University  of  Pennsylvania  Press. 
Philadelphia. 

2.  Elliot,  T.  R. 

1905.     J.  Physiol.  32:401. 

3.  Loewi,  O. 

1932-1933.     Harvey  Lectures.  218. 

4.  Brown,  G.  L. 

1937.     Physiol.  Rev.  17:485. 

5.  Eccles,  J.  C. 

1937.  Physiol.  Rev.  17:  538. 

6.  Fulton,  J.  F. 

1938, 1943.     Physiology  of  the  Nervous  System.    Oxford  Univ.  Press.    New  York. 

7.  Nachmansohn,  D. 

1945.  In:  Harris,  R.  S.,  &  K.V,  Thimann.     Vitamins  and  Hormones.    Aca- 
demic Press.     New  York.  3:  337. 

8.  Fulton,  J.  F.,  &  D.  Nachmansohn 
1943.     Science.  97:569. 

9.  Nachmansohn,  D. 

1946.  In:  Green,  D.  E.     Currents  in  Biochemical  Research.     Interscience  Pub- 
lishers.    New  York.  335. 

10.  Marnay,  A.,  &  D.  Nachmansohn 

1938.  J.  Physiol.  92:37. 

11.  Nachmansohn,  D. 

1940.     Yale  J.  Biol.  Med.  12:  565. 

12.  Boell,  E.  J.,  &  D.  Nachmansohn 
1940.     Science.  92:  513. 

13.  Nachmansohn,  D.,  H.  B.  Steinbach,  A.  L.  Machado,  &  S.  Spiegelman 
1943.     J.  Neurophysiol.  6:  203. 

14.  Hill,  A.  V. 

1932.     Chemical  Wave  Transmission  in  Nerve.     Cambridge  University  Press. 
London. 

15.  Nachmansohn,  D. 

1940.  Science.  91:  405. 

16.  Nachmansohn,  D.,  &  B.  Meyerhof 

1941.  J..  Neurophysiol.  4 :  348. 

17.  Nachmansohn,  D.,  R.  T.  Cox,  C.  W.  Coates,  &  A.  L.  Machado 
1913.     J.  Neurophysiol.  6:  203. 

18.  Nachmansohn,  D.,  C.  W.  Coates,  &  M.  A.  Rothenberg 
1946.     J.  Biol.  Chem.  163:  39. 

19.  Cole,  K.  C,  &  H.  T.  Curtis 

1939.  J.  Gen.  Physiol.  22:649. 

20.  Cox,  R.  T.,  C.  W.  Coate?,  &  M.  V.  Brown 

1945.     J.  Gen.  Physiol.  28:  187. 

21.  Eccles,  J.  C. 

1943.  J.  Physiol.  101:465. 

22.  Couteaux,  R. 

1944.  C.  R.  Soc.  biol.  138:976. 

23.  Couteaux,  R.,  &  D.  Nachmansohn 

1940.  Proc.  Soc.  Exp.  Biol.  &  Med.  43:  177. 


NACHMANSOHN:  CHEMICAL  MECHANISM  IN  NERVES       425 

24.  Couteauz,  R. 

1942.  Bull.  biol.  76:  14. 

26.  Nachznansohn,  D.,  &  M.  A.  Rothenberg 
1945.     J.  Biol.  Chem.  158:  653. 

26.  Richards,  A.  G..  &  L.  K.  Cutcomp 
1945.    J.  Cell.  Comp.  Physiol.  26:  57. 

27.  Nachmansohn,  D.,  &  A.  L.  Machado 

1943.  J.  Neurophysiol.  6:  397. 

28.  Nachmansohn,  D.,  H.  M.  John,  &  H.  Waelsch 

1943.  J.  Biol.  Chem.  150:  485. 

29.  Nachmansohn,  D.,  &  H.  M.  John 

1944.  Proc.  Soc.  Exp.  Biol.  &  Med.  57:  361. 

30.  Nachmansohn,  D.,  &  H.  M.  John 

1945.  J.  Biol.  Chem.  158:  157. 

31.  Ochoa,  J. 

1941.    J.  Biol.  Chem.  138:  751. 

32.  Nachmansohn,  D.,  &  M.  Berman 

1946.  J.  Biol.  Chem.    165:551 

33.  Nachmansohn,  D.,  &  H.  M.  John 

1945.  Science.  102:250, 

34.  Nachmansohn,  D.,  H.  M.  John,  &  M.  Berman 

1946.  J.  Biol.  Chem.  133:  475. 
36.  Lorente  de  No,  R. 

1944.  J.  Cell.  Comp.  Physiol.  24:  85. 

36.  Bullock,  T.  H.,  D.  Nachmansohn,  &  M.  A.  Rothenberg 

1946.     J.  Neurophysiol.  9:  9. 

37.  Michaelis,  L. 

1900.     Arch.  mikr.  Aiiat.  55:  565. 

38.  Feldborg,  W.,  A.  Fessard,  &  D.  Nachmansohn 
1940.    J.  Physiol.  97:  30. 

39.  King,  H. 

1935.    J.  Chem.  Soc.  London  2:  1381. 

40.  Wintersteiner,  O.,  &  J.  D.  Dutcher 

1943.     Science  97:  467. 

41.  Bullock,  T.  H.,  H.  Grundfest,  O.  Nachmansohn,  M.  A.  Rothenberg, 
&  K.  Sterling 

1946.    J.  Neurophysiol.  9:253. 

42.  von  Muralt,  A. 

1945.  Experiential:!. 


Annals  N.  Y.  Acad.  Sn. 


Vol.  XI.VII,  Akt.  4,  Pi.atk  -2 


5-6 


H  if-  -    ■-^■'■}fS^:  ' 


40-41  • 


55-56 


NACHMANSOHN;   CHKMICAL   MKCHANISM   OF   NKRVK  ACTIVITY 


NACHMANSOHN:  CHEMICAL  MECHANISM  IN  NERVES        427 


Plate  2 

Changes  of  the  morphological  structure  of  the  electric  plates  at  different  sec- 
tions of  the  electric  organ  of  Electrophorus  elcclricus. 

The  specimen  used  for  this  section  was  114  cm.  long.  All  sections  are  repro- 
duced with  the  same  magnification  (X  145).  The  numbers  below  each  section 
indicate  the  distance  in  cm.  from  the  anterior  end  of  the  organ.  H  —  bead  end, 
C  ~  caudal  end. 


428  ANNALS  NEW  YORK  ACADEMY  OF  SCIENCES 


Plate  3 

Changes  of  the  morphological  structure  of  the  electric  plates  at  different  sec- 
tions of  the  electric  organ  of  Electrophorus  electncus. 

The  specimen  used  for  this  section  was  57  cm.  long  (Xl45).  The  numbers 
below  each  section  indicate  the  distance  in  cm.  from  the  anterior  end  of  the  organ. 
H  =  head  end,  C  =  caudal  end. 


Annals  N.  Y.  Acad.  Sri. 


Vol..  XI.VII.  Aht.  4,  Plate  3 


H 


3-5 


NACHMANSOHN:   CHEMICAL   MKCHA.MSM   OK   NKRVF.   ACIIVITY 


.V 


AN  ELECTRICAL  HYPOTHESIS  OF  SYNAPTIC 
AND  NEURO-MUSCULAR  TRANSMISSION 

By  J.  C.  EccLES 

University  of  Otago,  Dunedin,  New  Zealand 

1.     PRESENT  THEORETICAL  POSITION 

This  paper  will  be  restricted  to  the  synapses  of  ganglia  and  the  spinal 
cord  and  to  the  neuro-muscular  junctions  of  skeletal  muscle  (hence- 
forth, collectively  referred  to  as  synapses),  because,  physiologically, 
they  form  a  fairly  homogeneous  group.  A  preliminary  report  has  al- 
ready been  published.^*  There  is  now  good  evidence  that  the  trans- 
mission of  impulses,  at  all  these  synapses,  is  mediated  by  catelectrotonic 
potentials  set  up  at  the  synaptic  membrane  of  the  post-synaptic  cell — 
the  end-plate  potentials  of  skeletal  muscle-^'  ^^'  ^*'  ^^'  ^°'  "  and  the 
synaptic  potentials  of  ganglion  cells^"'  ^^  and  motoneurones.^'  ^^'  ^^  We 
may,  therefore,  subdivide  the  problem  of  synaptic  transmission  into 
two  Problems:  (a)  the  mechanism  whereby  impulses  in  pre-synaptic 
nerve  fibers  set  up  catelectrotonic  synaptic  potentials  in  the  post- 
synaptic cell;  and  (b)  the  initiation  of  impulses  in  the  post-synaptic 
cell  by  these  synaptic  potentials.  As  is  well  known,  the  existing  hy- 
potheses relating  to  Problem  (a)  are  chemical  (acetylcholine),  or  elec- 
trical, or  some  combination  thereof.*'  -'''  *^^  Problem  (b)  has,  hitherto, 
been  regarded  as  just  a  part  of  the  general  problem  of  impulse  initiation 
by  catelectrotonus.  However,  there  is  evidence  of  a  unique  mechanism 
in  the  case  of  the  only  synapse  worked  on  in  detail.^" 

There  is  some  resemblance  between  these  two  stages  of  synaptic 
transmission  and  the  two  "boundary  faces"  postulated  by  Buchthal  and 
Lindhard,^'  ^  to  explain  the  two  stages  of  neuro-muscular  block  pro- 
duced by  curare  and  acetylcholine. 

In  their  existing  form,  both  hypotheses  relating  to  Problem  (a)  are 
unsatisfactory : 

(i)  Originally,  the  acetylcholine  hypothesis  simply  stated  that  a  pre- 
synaptic impulse  liberated  at  the  synapse  a  sudden  jet  of  acetylcholine, 
which  excited  the  post-synaptic  cell  by  acting  on  specific  receptors  ;^^ 
thus  set  up  the  synaptic  potential,  according  to  present  views;  and 
was  ickly  removed  by  the  locally  concentrated  cholinesterase.®-  ^'  "•  ^^ 
The  usual  failure  to  detect  acetylcholine  in  venous  blood  collected  from 

(429) 


430  ANNALS  NEW  YORK  ACADEMY  OF  SCIENCES 

eserinized,  stimulated  ganglia  or  muscle  suggested  the  additional  hy- 
pothesis that,  normally,  acetylcholine  is  removed  by  being  rebuilt 
rapidly  to  a  precursor  and  that  cholinesterase  merely  acts  as  a  barrier, 
to  prevent  diffusion  of  acetylcholine  away  from  the  synapse.**^  Further 
additions  to  this  hypothesis  were  needed,  in  order  to  explain  the  effects 
of  eserine  and  curarine  on  end-plate  potentials.*  It  is  unsatisfactory 
that  the  acetylcholine  hypothesis  has  had  to  be  reconciled  with  new 
experimental  evidence,  by  thus  making  subsidiary  ad  hoc  hypotheses, 
which  have  not  been  independently  testable.  The  most  recent  develop- 
ment of  the  acetylcholine  hypothesis*'^  is  essentially  a  special  type  of 
the  electrical  hypothesis,  for  it  postulates  that  electrical  transmission 
across  the  synapse  excites  the  postsynaptic  liberation  of  acetylcholine, 
which,  in  turn,  sets  up  the  synaptic  potential. 

(ii)  Most  expressions  of  the  electrical  hypothesis  of  synaptic  trans- 
mission have  merely  stated  that  the  electrical  currents  of  the  pre- 
synaptic impulses  set  up  impulses  in  the  post-synaptic  cell,  much  as  one 
segment  of  a  nerve  excites  the  next.^°'  ^^'  ^°'  ^^'  '^^  The  attempt  at  pre- 
cise formulation  by  Lapicque  (the  isochronism  hypothesis)  has  had 
to  be  modified  so  much  by  the  recognition  of  the  significance  of  addi- 
tional factors  (relative  durations  of  the  pre-  and  post-synaptic  re- 
sponses,"'^ the  rheobase  of  the  post-synaptic  celP^) ,  that  it  now  states 
little  more  than  the  above  vague  formulation.  Thus,  the  electrical 
hypothesis  is  unsatisfactory  (indeed,  virtually  useless),  because  it  is 
so  vaguely  expressed  that  it  fails  to  give  predictions  that  would  be  a 
fertile  source  of  experimental  tests. 

2.     RECENT  EXPERIMENTAL  SUPPORT  FOR  AN 
ELECTRICAL  HYPOTHESIS 

The  need  for  a  more  developed  electrical  hypothesis  has  now  become 
urgent,  because  the  following  recent  investigations  have  indicated  that 
acetylcholine  plays  but  a  subsidiary  role  at  ganglionic  synapses,  and  a 
negligible  role  at  spinal  cord  synapses.  With  muscle,  too,  there  are 
indications  that  electrical  transmission  may  play  an  important  part. 

(i)  A  detailed  study^- -'^  of  the  electrical  responses  of  eserinized 
ganglia  (normal  and  curarized)  revealed  an  excitatory  action,  pro- 
longed for  several  seconds  after  repetitive  stimulation  and  attributable 
to  acetylcholine.  However,  this  prolonged  action  was  so  weak  that 
the  summation  of  about  20  volleys  in  (juick  succession  was  needed  to 
make  it  sufficiently  strong  to  excite  normal,  fully  eserinized  ganglion 


•  Cf.  Eccles,  J.  C,  B.  Katz,  &  S.  W.  Kuffler.":  227-8. 


ECCLES:    ELECTRICAL    THEORIES   OF    TRANSMISSION        431 

cells  to  discharge.  The  usual  transmission  mechanism  was  due  to  an 
excitatory  action,  unaffected  by  eserine  and  brief  enough  to  be  at- 
tributable to  the  action  currents  of  the  pre-ganglionic  impulses. ^^'  '^ 

(ii)  A  similar  study  of  synaptic  potentials  of  motoneurones  excited 
through  mono-synaptic  reflex  pathways  of  the  spinal  cord  (frog,  cat) 
has  failed  to  detect  even  such  a  subsidiary  role  for  acetylcholine  trans- 
mission.^^ Furthermore,  it  has  been  found  that  synaptic  transmission 
of  the  frog's  spinal  cord  is  unaffected  by  prolonged  soaking  (several 
hours)  in  high  concentrations  of  acetylcholine  (up  to  1  in  5,000) .  Still 
higher  concentrations  have  an  anesthetic  action  which,  initially,  is 
reversible.  The  isolated  oxygenated  cord  (anesthetized  or  unanes- 
thetized)  is  soaked  for  30  min.  in  a  strong  anti-cholinesterase  (1  in 
50,000  eserine),  and  then  the  acetylcholine  is  added  to  the  solution." 
These  experiments  would  appear  to  falsify  the  hypothesis  that  acetyl- 
choline plays  a  major  role  in  synaptic  transmission  in  the  spinal  cord. 
However,  too  much  emphasis  should  not  be  placed  on  these  latter  ex- 
periments, until  they  are  repeated  with  prostigraine  as  an  anti-cholin- 
esterase (cf.  iii,  below). 

(iii)  Just  as  with  sympathetic  ganglia,^''  the  responses  of  curarized, 
eserinized  (or  prostigminized)  muscles  to  repetitive  stimulation  are 
sharply  distinguishable  into  a  prolonged  end-plate  potential  which  is 
attributable  to  acetylcholine,  and  an  initial,  very  brief,  end-plate 
potential,  but  little  lengthened  by  anti-cholinesterases.^^'  ^'^  It  seems 
probable  that,  as  with  ganglia,  the  small,  apparent  lengthening  of  the 
initial  phase  by  anti-cholinesterases  may  be  attributable  to  some  ad- 
mixture of  the  prolonged  acetylcholine  phase,  and  that  the  initial  phase 
may  be  excited  by  the  action  currents  of  pre-synaptic  impulses  (cf. 
PART  8,  ii).  Acetylcholine  blocks  neuro-muscular  transmission,^'  ^  pre- 
sumably by  catelectrotonic  blockage,  but,  despite  a  relatively  high 
acetylcholine  background  (1  in  200,000),  pre-synaptic  volleys  still  set 
up  large  end-plate  potentials,  even  larger  than  in  curarized  muscle.^® 
When  performing  these  experiments  by  soaking  frog's  sartorii  in  acetyl- 
choline solutions,  prostigmine  is  used  as  an  anti-cholinesterase.  Eser- 
ine is  ineffective,  probably  because  acetylcholine  competes  with  it  for 
the  cholinesterase."* 

3.     EXPERIMENTAL  BASIS  FOR  ELECTRICAL  HYPOTHESIS 

In  recent  years,  important  advances  have  been  made  in  the  investiga- 
tion of  nerve  and  muscle  fibers,  and  an  electrical  hypothesis  of  trans- 
mission must  be  based  on  the  following  evidence: 


*  Cf.  Eccles,  J.  C,  B.  Katz,  8c  S.  W.  Kufller.^^:  225. 


432 


ANNALS  NEW  YORK  ACADEMY  OF  SCIENCES 


A.     The  Electrical  Properties  of  the   Surface   Membranes  and   the 

Changes   Produced   by    Catelectrotonus,    Anelectrotonus, 

Local  Responses,  and  Propagated  Impulses 

Quantitative  measurements  liave  been  made  of  resistance,  electro- 
motive force,  capacity,  and  rectification.  The  great  diminution  of  the 
two  former  during  the  excited  phase  of  the  impulse  has  been  described 
for  nerve  and  muscle,  vertebrate  and  invertebrate.^'  ^^'  "'  ^^'  ^^'  ^^'  **''  *'' 
There  is  no  good  evidence  that  the  large  inductance  of  cephalopod 
nerve^"'  ^*  is  present  in  normal  vertebrate  nerve  or  muscle.  The  phe- 
nomenon of  para-resonance  is  simply  explained  in  terms  of  the  two 
excitation  constants  of  nerve.*  In  contrast  with  cephalopod  nerve, 
there  is,  in  frog  muscle,  no  appreciable  lag  between  a  sudden  change 
in  potential  and  the  associated  change  in  the  resistance  of  the  mem- 
brane.*^ Such  a  lag  in  cephalopod  nerve  has  been  attributed  to  the 
large  membrane  inductance.^-  On  present  evidence,  vertebrate  nerve 
and  muscle  may,  therefore,  be  regarded  as  having  a  negligible  induc- 
tance, and  may  be  provisionally  schematized,  as  in  figure  1. 


Figure  L  Diagram  showing  probable  electrical  characteristics  of  ner/e  and  muscle  membranes, 
rx  and  n  being,  respectively,  the  external  and  internal  longitudinal  resistances ;  C,  E,  and  R,  the 
capacity,  battery,  and  resistance  of  the  membrane. 


B.     Local  Responses 

All  grades  of  active  local  responses,  short  of  propagated,  all-or- 
nothing  impulses,  have  been  shown  to  exist  in  nerve  and  muscle,^'  ^'''  ^^' 
44, 4G,  47, 52, 70,  79  q^^^  jjj^y  j^g  explalucd  as  due  to  the  limited  area  ex- 
cited"® and/or  to  the  low  intensity  of  the  excitation. ^^  In  refractory, 
anesthetized,  or  deteriorated  nerve  or  muscle,  these  local  responses  may 
be  very  large.*^'  *'■  ®^'  ^°'  ''^  It  appears  probable  that  active  local  re- 
sponses differ  from  passive  electrotonic  changes  (including  rectifica- 
tion), jiist  as  with  the  propagated  impulse,  in  that  they  are  caused  by  a 
temporary  diminution,  extinction,  or  even  reversal,  of  the  membrane 
battery.^'  i^'  "•  '^ 


*  Katz,  B.«:  28. 


ECCLES:  ELECTRICAL  THEORIES  OF  TRANSMISSION 


433 


C.     Reactions  of  Ephapses  (Artificial  Synapses) 

The  double  axon  preparations^'  ■**'  *^  are  particularly  relevant  to 
electrical  action  across  synapses.  Since  there  has  been  excellent  corre- 
lation between  the  effects  predicted  by  the  "local  current"  theory  of 
nerve  conduction  and  the  effects  observed,  it  may  be  concluded  that 
these  effects  are  caused  by  electrical  current  flow  across  the  ephapse. 
48, 49, 64  According  to  the  geometry  of  the  ephaptic  contact,  three  main 
types  of  effect  are  exerted  on  the  resting  fiber  by  an  impulse  in  the  ac- 
tive fiber :^ 

(i)  At  regions  where  fibers  are  contiguous  for  some  distance  on 
either  side — for  maximum  effect,  at  least  half  a  wave  length.  Here 
the  currents  generated  by  the  impulse  have,  in  turn,  anodal,  cathodal, 
and  anodal  action  on  the  resting  fiber.*^*  ''^     Figure  2  shows  that  the 


Figure  2.  Diagram  of  two  contiguous  fibers,  showing  the  current  flow  generated  by  impulse  in 
active  lower  fiber  and  its  penetration  of  the  resting  fiber  (cf.  Katz  &  Schmitt,  figure  2''*).  As  the 
impulse  (shown  above)  propagates  along  the  active  fiber,  any  point  on  the  resting  fiber  is  sub- 
jected, in  turn,  to  effects  A1C1C2A2.  Active  part  of  impulse  shown  by  hatched  area  in  this  and 
subsequent  figures. 

cathodal  phase  is  really  double,  being  due  to  currents  generated  by  the 
membrane  battery  of  the  active  fiber,  at  first  ahead  (Ci),  and  then  in 
the  wake,  of  the  impulse  (Ca).  Thus,  the  sequence  of  action  is 
A1C1C2A2,  as  the  impulse  sweeps  past  a  point  on  the  resting  fiber.  As 
Katz  and  Schmitt  pointed  out,  the  current  penetrating  and  acting  on 
the  resting  fiber  is  virtually  a  mirror  image  of  the  penetrating  current 
of  the  active  fiber,  and  hence,  has  an  intensity-time  course  correspond- 
ing to  the  second  derivative  of  the  monophasic  action  potential,  with 
respect  to  the  time  coordinate  (d^P/dt^).  Since  the  curvature  of  the 
wave  front  of  the  impulse  is  at  least  twice  as  sharp  as  that  of  its  wake, 
Ai  and  Ci  will  be  at  least  twice  as  large  as  C2  and  A2, 
will  be  correspondingly  longer  in  duration.* 


•  Cf.  Katz,  B.,  ft  O.  H.  Sdunltt.*^     Ftgijees  5Aji  and  6,  Curve  1. 


434 


ANNALS  NEW  YORK  ACADEMY  OF  SCIENCES 


(ii)  At  regions  where  the  resting  fiber  is  not  affected  by  the  approach 
of  the  impulse,  but  only  by  its  immediate  juxtaposition  and  its  propaga- 
tion. As  shown  in  figure  3a,  this  occurs  when  the  impulse  propagates 
from  an  electrically  insulated  region  of  the  active  fiber  to  a  region 
where  it  is  in  contiguity  with  the  resting  fiber.  Effects  Ai  and  C2  are 
prevented  by  the  insulation,  the  interaction  being  due  to  effects  Ci  and 


Figure  3a.  Diagram  showing  current  flow  at  junctional  zone  of  two  previously  separated  fibers, 
(i)  Impulse  at  junction  give.s  Ci  effect  on  resting  fiber;  (ii)  after  further  propagation,  wake  of 
impulse  gives  A2  effect. 


(i) 


(ii) 


#i^^ 


se 


E*==F 


air. 


w^- 


Figure  3b.  Penetrating  current  generated  by  impulse  arising  in  one  fiber,  (i)  Ci  effect,  as  im- 
pulse is  initiated ;  (ii)  A2  effect,  in  wake  of  impulse,  propagating  in  both  directions  from  site  of 
origin. 


(i) 


(ii) 


%' nCi 

1 

1 

1 
1 

\ 

^//// 

<f- 


C^- N^g 

1 
1 

A 
1 

1 

y//^ 

_    _/ 

Figure  3c.  Two  fibers  connected  by  double  salt  bridge,  (i)  Impulse  opposite  proximal  arm; 
(ii)  impulse  opposite  distal  arm,  showing  C1A2  action  on  resting  fiber  at  proximal  arm  and  A1C2 
action  at  distal  arm. 


A2*  A  similar  effect  would  also  be  produced  at  a  region  in  a  passive 
fiber  adjacent  to  the  origin  of  the  impulse  in  the  active  fiber  (figure 
3b),  and  at  the  proximal  arm  of  a  double  salt  bridge  (figure  3c). ^^ 

(iii)   At  regions  where  the  resting  fiber  is  influenced  by  the  approach 
and  juxtaposition  of  an  impulse,  but  not  by  its  propagation.     Figure  4 


*  Cf .  Arvanltakl,  A.'     Figure  4  III ;  Marrazzi,  A.  S..  8t  R.  Iiorente  de  XTd.^"     Figure  3. 


ECCLES:  ELECTRICAL  THEORIES  OF  TRANSMISSION         435 

(also  FIGURE  3c,  for  distal  arm  of  double  salt  bridge)  shows  that  this 
occurs  under  the  converse  conditions  to  those  above,  and  that  effects 


C2.^-~      ~  N 


e^^?^^ 


Figure  4a.  Converse  diagram  to  figure  3a,  showing  current  flow  at  zone  of  separation  of  two 
previously  contiguous  fibers,  fi)  Impulse  approaching  bifurcation  gives  Ai  effect;  (ii)  impulse  at 
bifurcation  gives  C2  effect. 


(i) 


(ii) 


t'^l  ""^'  ^>i^  ^^^, 


r^W^ 


3E 


&K 


Figure  4b.     (i)   Two  impulses  approaching  a  collision  give  Ai  effect  on  resting  fiber;   and   (ii) 
at  collision  of  impulses,  02  effect  on  resting  fiber. 

Ci  and  A2  are  prevented,  the  action  being  due  to  effects  Ai  and  C2.* 
At  synaptic  regions,  a  similar  electrical  action  would  be  exerted  by  the 
pre-synaptic  impulse  on  the  post-synaptic  membrane  (cf.  figure  5), 
since  the  surrounding  conducting  medium  provides  a  pathway  for  cur- 
rents generated  by  the  approaching  impulse.  There  is,  of  course,  no 
departing  impulse  {boutons  de  passage  excepted).  In  this  connection, 
Arvanitaki's  results^  are  of  especial  interest,  for  it  was  only  in  ephaptic 
situations  giving  A1C2  effects  that  she  observed  appreciable  local  re- 
sponses of  the  resting  fiber.  She  concluded  that,  in  all  other  condi- 
tions, the  terminal  A,  effect  suppressed  any  active  response  of  the  rest- 
ing fiber. 

The  approximate  time-course  of  the  penetrating  current  at  a  synapse 
may  be  derived  from  the  monophasic  spike  potential  at  the  pre-synaptic 
terminal,  by  considering  the  flow  of  current,  much  as  Katz  and  Schmitt*^ 
did  for  two  parallel  fibers.  The  longitudinal  current  flowing  in  the 
external  circuit  is  proportional  to  the  first  derivative  of  the  monophasic 
potential,  dP/dt.  Immediately  proximal  to  the  end  of  the  pre-synaptic 
fiber,  all  the  longitudinal  current  is  provided  by  the  current  penetrating 
the  terminal  end.  Hence,  this  penetrating  current  is  also  proportional  to 
dP/dt,t  and  not  to  d'P/dt^  as  occurs  along  the  length  of  the  fiber.     A 

*  Cf.  Arvanitakl,  A.^     Figure  4  IT. 

t  Cf.  Marrazzl,  A.  S.,  &  XI.  Iiorente  de  JSf6^:  89. 


436 


ANNALS  NEW  YORK  ACADEMY  OF  SCIENCES 


similar  time-course  may  be  assumed  for  the  current  penetrating  the 
closely  adjacent  post-synaptic  membrane  (see  figure  5).  The  first 
derivative  of  the  monophasic  potential  gives,  of  course,  the  expected 
diphasic  effect,  A1C2. 

Thus,  it  may  be  concluded  that,  so  far  as  they  go,  ephaptic  investi- 
gations lend  support  to  the  hypothesis  that  an  excitatory  action  would 
be  exerted  by  impulses  terminating  at  synapses.  However,  with 
ephapses,  this  excitatory  action  is  normally  too  weak  to  initiate  im- 
pulses in  the  resting  fiber.  For  example,  Katz  and  Schmitt*^  find  that 
the  maximum  C  effect  is  never  as  much  as  20%  of  threshold,  and 
Arvanitaki^  has  to  sensitize  the  resting  fiber  by  decalcification,  in 
order  to  increase  the  local  response  sufficiently  for  impulse  initiation.* 


/ 


T^ 


\^  ^ 


/ 


(a) 


(b) 

Figure  5.  Diagrams  of  current  flow  at  a  schematic  synapse  with  pre-synaptic  impulse  ap- 
proaching synapse  in  (a),  and  at  synapse  in  (b).  Note  reversal  of  current  flow,  the  focal  Ai 
effect  being  followed  by  the  focal  C2  effect  at  the  synaptic  region  of  the  post-synaptic  membrane. 

The  Hering  effect,  ephaptic  transmission  adjacent  to  killed  or  injured 
regions,^^'  *^'  "  may  also  be  explained  as  due  to  A1C2  stimulation  of 
fibers  rendered  sensitive  by  the  catelectrotonus'*  prevailing  close  to  the 
injured  region.     Tests  of  excitability  changes,  4  ram.  from  the  killed 

*  Cf .  Arvanitaki,  A.>     Figure  S. 


ECCLES:  ELECTRICAL  THEORIES  OF  TRANSMISSION         437 

end,  showed  a  terminal  A2  effect,^*  but,  presumably,  this  would  dis- 
appear, closer  to  the  killed  end.  The  ephaptic  experiments,  in  general, 
show  that  special  conditions  must  prevail  at  synaptic  contacts,  if  elec- 
trical excitation  is  to  be  adequate  for  synaptic  transmission  (cf.  part  8) . 

D.     Special  Properties  of  the  Synaptic  Region 

So  far,  such  investigation  has  been  restricted  to  the  isolated  neuro- 
muscular junction.  When  electrical  recording  is  effectively  localized 
to  the  end-plate  region  of  the  muscle,  it  has  been  shown  that  the  end- 
plate  potential  set  up  by  a  nerve  impulse  rises  smoothly  to  the  full 
height  of  the  spike  potential,''"  w^ithout  showing  the  sudden  inflection 
characteristic  of  impulse  initiation.*^-  ^-'  "^  The  impulse  appears  to 
be  initiated,  a  little  later,  by  an  adjacent  region  of  the  membrane, 
when  it  reaches  a  critical  intensity  of  catelectrotonus.  Progressive 
curarization  progressively  diminishes  the  end-plate  potential;  the  im- 
pulse initiation  occurs  adjacently,  after  the  longer  delay  ensuing  before 
the  lower  end-plate  potential  builds  up  the  critical  catelectrotonus ;  and 
eventually,  transmission  fails.  It  may,  therefore,  be  assumed  that  the 
end-plate  region  of  the  muscle  is  speciahzed  to  give  "local  responses" 
of  high  and  graduated  intensities,  without  the  sudden  incursion  of  the 
all-or-nothing  "breakdown"  of  resistance  and  battery  that  occurs  with 
impulse  initiation. ^°  This  evidence  of  unique  electrical  properties 
of  the  end-plate  is  relatable  to  its  well-known,  unique,  pharmacological 
properties. ^^'  ^*'  ^^  In  the  isolated  preparation,  Kuffler^^  has  failed  to 
detect  the  large  resting  potential  (positive  or  negative)  between  the 
surface  of  the  end-plate  and  that  of  the  muscle  fiber  that  has  been 
described  by  Buchthal  and  Lindhard.^ 

4.     INITIAL  ASSUMPTIONS  OF  HYPOTHESIS 

The  following  three  initial  assumptions  of  the  electrical  hypothesis 
are  based  on  the  evidence  of  the  preceding  four  sections,  together  with 
the  conventional  histological  picture  (they  form,  as  it  were,  a  model 
of  a  synapse  whose  functional  operation  will  be  discussed  in  part  5) : 

A.  That  the  geometrical  situation  at  the  synapse  may  be  schematic- 
ally represented  by  the  pre-synaptic  fiber  ending  as  a  cylindrical  mem- 
brane, with  a  closed  end  in  close  apposition  to  the  large  plane  surface 
membrane  of  the  post-synaptic  cell,  as  is  shown  in  section  in  figure  5. 
Histologists  are  now  fairly  generally  agreed  that  a  transverse  mem- 
brane exists  at  the  synapse,^"  and  there  is  also  electrical  evidence*  of  a 


*  Eccles,  J.  C.2»:  352. 


438  ANNALS  NEW  YORK  ACADEMY  OF  SCIENCES 

highly  resistant  transverse  membrane.     This  evidence  for  ganglionic 
synapses  also  obtains  for  neuro-muscular  junctions. 

B.  That,  in  general,  the  surface  membranes  of  figure  5  have  the 
electrical  properties  demonstrated  for  peripheral  nerve  and  muscle 
membranes:  resistance,  electromotive  force,  capacity,  and  rectification, 
as  shown  in  figure  1.  There  are  no  direct  observations  on  nerve  cells, 
but  they  resemble  nerve  fibers  in  their  electrical  excitability  and  in 
the  propagation  of  impulses  from  a  nerve  cell  to  its  axon,^'''  ^^'  **°  and 
vice  versa. ^°'  ^^'  *^'''  "^  It  may  also  be  assumed  that  both  the  exterior 
and  the  interior  of  the  cells  are  good  conducting  media,  and  that  the 
resting  potential  of  the  post-synaptic  membrane  is  identical  with  that 
for  the  remainder  of  the  post-synaptic  cell.^^ 

C.  That  the  synaptic  region  of  the  post-synaptic  cell  has  unique 
electrical  properties,  in  that  cathodal  polarization  (lowering  of  resting 
charge)  sets  up  a  graduated  "local  response,"  with  a  temporarily  ir- 
reversible and  large  diminution  of  electromotive  force  and  resistance, 
but  not  the  all-or-nothing  membrane  "breakdown"  characteristic  of  the 
propagated  impulse  (cf.  part  3,  B  and  D).  Direct  evidence  is  only 
available  for  the  end-plate  region,^"  but  the  assumption  is  extended  to 
the  synaptic  regions  of  nerve  cells. 

5.     DEVELOPMENT  OF  HYPOTHESIS  ON  THE  BASIS 

OF  THIS  MODEL 

It  appears  that,  assuming  A  and  B,  we  have  to  expect  that  the  cur- 
rent generated  by  an  impulse  propagating  up  to  the  terminal  of  the  pre- 
synaptic fiber  will,  in  part,  penetrate  the  post-synaptic  cell  and  give 
a  diphasic  action  (cf.  part  3,  C,  iii).  Firstly,  there  will  be  an  anodal 
focus,  Ai,  at  the  synaptic  region,  with  a  cathodal  surround  (figures 
5a  and  6a).  Then,  when  the  active  region  of  the  impulse  reaches  the 
terminal,  current  flow  will  reverse,  giving  a  cathodal  focus,  C2,  with  an 
anodal  surround  (figures  5b  and  6b).  The  penetrating  current  will 
be  limited  by  polarization  of  the  membrane  and,  in  the  initial  phase, 
by  the  increasing  resistance  of  the  localized  anode  (rectification  effect) . 
On  account  of  its  much  larger  area,  the  membrane  resistance  (and  pene- 
trating current  density)  at  the  cathodal  surround  will  be  so  much  lower 
that  its  simultaneous  diminution  by  the  rectification  effect  will  be 
relatively  insignificant  in  tending  to  increase  the  flow  of  penetrating 
current.  However,  in  the  second  phase,  the  situation  is  reversed,  be- 
cause, on  account  of  the  high  current  density,  the  lowering  of  the  initi- 
ally high  resistance  at  the  localized  cathode  will  have  a  preponderant 
effect  in  increasing  the  flow  of  current.     Hence,  due  to  rectification,  the 


ECCLES:  ELECTRICAL  THEORIES  OF  TRANSMISSION         439 

over-all  resistance  offered  to  the  penetrating  current  will  be  much  lower 
in  the  second  phase  than  in  the  first,  the  current  being,  as  it  were, 
canalized  through  the  localized  low  resistance  at  the  cathodal  focus. 
It  should  be  noted  that,  in  this  way,  rectification  will  diminish,  at  the 
synapse,  the  depressing  action  of  the  relatively  high  intensity  Ai,  and 
then  increase  the  stimulating  action  of  the  relatively  low  intensity  Cz 
(cf.  FIGURE  7b).  The  effectiveness  of  this  discriminative  action  of 
rectification  is  illustrated  for  bipolar  stimulation,  by  Cole.*  It  should 
be  even  more  effective  for  the  unipolar  type  of  stimulation  that  occurs 
at  the  synapse.  It  is  evident  that,  if  the  membrane  had  a  high  induct- 
ance in  series  with  R  (figure  1),  the  brief  penetrating  currents  would 
be  much  less  intense,  and  the  rectification  correspondingly  less  effective. 
The  polarization  of  the  membrane,  in  the  first  and  second  stages,  is 
shown  diagrammatically  in  figures  6a  and  6b.  Note  the  wider  spread 
of  anelectrotonus,  Ai,  than  C2,  and  the  reversal  of  potential  gradients 
along  the  inner  side  of  the  membrane,  corresponding  to  the  reversal  of 
the  "core  currents"  (cf.  figures  5a  and  5b).  Note,  also,  that,  at  the 
dotted  lines  separating  the  anelectrotonic  and  catelectrotonic  areas,  the 
curves  of  the  inner  and  outer  membrane  potentials  are  inflected,  as 
would  be  expected  for  zero  density  of  penetrating  current.  The  catelec- 
trotonic focus  shown  in  figure  6b  will  not  immediately  develop  the 
pre-synaptic  current  flow  reverses.  The  anodal  polarization  in  figure 
6a  takes  some  time  to  be  removed  by  the  local  current  flow,  as  well  as 
by  the  reversed  penetrating  currents,  and  further  time  is  needed  to 
charge  the  membrane  condensers  to  the  fully-developed  cathodal  focus 
in  figure  6b  (cf.  figure  7b).  If,  at  this  latter  stage,  the  external  elec- 
tric field,  applied  by  the  impulse  in  the  pre-synaptic  terminal,  were  sud- 
denly removed,  the  membrane  would  immediately  revert  to  the  poten- 
tial distribution  of  figure  6c  (assuming  that  the  internal  and  external 
media  have  equal  longitudinal  resistances;  i.e.,  that  ri  and  r2  of  figure 
1  are  equal).  If  no  local  response  is  set  up,  i.e.,  if  the  membrane  ex- 
hibits only  its  electrotonic  properties,  local  current  flow  would  quickly 
cause  the  anodal  surround  to  discharge  into,  and  repolarize,  the 
cathodal  focus,  and  the  membrane  would  quickly  revert  to  the  normal, 
uniformly  charged,  condition.  Thus,  under  such  circumstances,  with 
the  usual  disposition  of  electrodes  for  recording  responses  at  the 
synaptic  region  (one  close  to  the  synapse  and  one  distally  on  the  post- 
synaptic cell) ,  there  would  be  recorded  merely  a  brief  diphasic  poten- 
tial, attributable  to  currents  generated  by  the  pre-synaptic  impulse  and 
but  little  modified  by  the  passive  properties  of  the  post-synaptic  cell. 

*  Cole.  K.  S."     FiCDKE  3. 


440 


ANNALS  NEW  YORK  ACADEMY  OF  SCIENCES 


(a) 


(b) 


(c) 


(d) 

Figure  6.  Graphs  of  spatial  distribution  of  potentials  on  the  outer  and  inner  sides  of  post- 
synaptic membrane,  the  synaptic  region  being  in  center,  i.e.,  potentials  are  ordinates  and  dis- 
tances abscissae.  The  reference  potential  is  given  by  a  distal  region  of  the  membrane,  the  outer 
side  being  shown  above  the  inner.  The  normal  resting  condition  is  shown  by  the  broken  lines 
separated  by  the  resting  potential,  (a)  Initial  Ai  focus  at  synapse  with  low  intensity  cathodal  sur- 
round, (b)  Reversed  phase  with  C2  synaptic  focus  and  anodal  surround  (cf.  figure  5b).  (c) 
Membrane  potentials,  when  external  field  generated  by  pre-synaptic  impulse  is  removed,  the 
spatial  distribution  of  the  potentials  across  the  membrane  being  identical  with  those  of  (b). 
(d)  Potentials  after  generation  of  local  response  at  synapse,  with  the  catelectrotonus  (the  synap- 
tic potential)  spreading  thence  over  the  post-synaptic  membrane.  Hatched  area  shows  specialized 
synaptic  zone  of  membrane.     Impulse  initiation  occurs  outside  this  zone,  for  example  at  the  arrow. 


There  would  be  no  synaptic  potential,  with  its  characteristic  long 
duration. 

The  additional  assumption  (part  4,  C)  is  necessary,  in  order  to  ex- 
plain  the  origin  of  the  synaptic  potential.  It  postulates  that,  when 
above  a  critical  intensity,  the  cathodal  focus  evokes  at  the  synaptic 


ECCLES:  ELECTRICAL  THEORIES  OF  TRANSMISSION         441 

region  of  the  post-synaptic  cell  an  intense  ''local  response,"  which  runs 
through  a  cycle  of  increasing  and  decreasing  intensity,  much  as  does 
the  spike  of  a  propagating  impulse.''^'  ^^'  ^^^  ^°'  '^  This  local  response, 
thus,  would  outlast  the  second  phase  of  the  penetrating  current-flow, 
and  provide  a  relatively  enduring  focus,  of  very  low  polarization  (pre- 
sumably due  to  diminution  or  extinction  of  the  membrane  battery)  and 
resistance,  through  which  adjacent  regions  of  the  post-synaptic  mem- 
brane proceed  to  discharge.  Such  a  local  response  is  actually  ob- 
served at  an  ephapse  giving  A1C2  interaction.^  Figuee  6d  shows  the 
internal  and  external  potentials  of  a  fully-developed  local  response  at 
the  end-plate  region,  where  Kuffler^°  finds  the  potential  as  high  as  the 
spike  potential.  It  is  shown  as  zero  transverse  membrane  potential, 
since  it  is  not  known  if  reversal  of  potential  occurs  with  the  muscle 
spike.  Also,  in  figure  6d,  the  anodal  surround  (of  figure  6c)  has 
given  place  to  a  catelectrotonic  surround  of  diminished  polarization, 
which  spreads  spatially,  according  to  "core  conductor"  theory.  With 
muscle,  it  appears  that  the  all-or-nothing  spike  arises  when  the  mem- 
brane adjacent  to  the  end-plate  is  critically  depolarized,^"  e.g.,  at  the 
arrow  (figure  6d).  The  synapses  of  ganglion  cells  and  of  moto- 
neurones  of  the  spinal  cord  have  also  been  observed  to  generate  such 
catelectrotonic  potentials  (synaptic  potentials),  spreading  spatially,  ac- 
cording to  core  conductor  theory.-'  ^^'  ^^  It  has  further  been  shown 
that,  as  with  the  end-plate  potential,^*'-  "  these  potentials  have  a  time- 
course,  which  may  be  interpreted  as  due  to  a  brief,  active  polarization 
and  a  passive  exponential  decay  governed  by  the  electric  time  con- 
stant (the  product  CR,  in  figure  1)  of  the  membrane.^-'  -^  In  the 
present  hypothesis,  the  active  depolarizing  action  is  provided  by  the 
local  response  of  the  synaptic  region  of  the  post-synaptic  cell,  not 
directly  by  the  currents  generated  by  the  pre-synaptic  impulse,  as  has 
hitherto  been  assumed  in  hypotheses  of  electrical  transmission.^"'  ^^'  ®^' 
77, 78  There  have,  however,  been  suggestions  of  a  possible  involvement 
of  a  local  response.* 

The  recent  hypothesis  of  Nachmansohn*'^  is  relevant  to  assumption 
C,  for  it  would  postulate  that  the  local  response  is  due  to  the  action  of 
acetylcholine  liberated  by  the  post-synaptic  membrane  at  a  critical  in- 
tensity of  catelectrotonus  {i.e.,  at  X  in  figure  7c).  Such  an  assump- 
tion is  readily  assimilable  to  the  present  electrical  hypothesis,  but  its 
general  application  to  nerve  impulse  transmission  would  seem  to  be 
falsified  by  Lorente  de  No's  finding^^  that  this  transmission  is  unaf- 
fected by  high  concentrations  of  acetylcholine. 


*  Eccles,  J.  C.21:  369;  Lorente  de  N6,  B.««:  449;  Arvanltaki,  A.^:  103. 


442  ANNALS  NEW  YORK  ACADEMY  OF  SCIENCES 

The  further  problem  of  synaptic  transmission  concerns  the  initia- 
tion of  propagated  impulses  by  the  catelectrotonic  synaptic  potential 
(see  PART  1,  Problem  (b)).  The  observations  of  Kuffler'"  on  the  iso- 
lated neuro-muscular  junction  indicate  that  the  initiation  of  the  im- 
pulse occurs  in  the  muscle  membrane  adjacent  to  the  end-plate  region 
(cf.  FIGURE  6d).  No  such  intimate  observations  have  been  made  for 
synapses  in  ganglia  and  the  spinal  cord,  but  it  may  be  assumed  that 
the  catelectrotonic  potentials  from  diverse  synaptic  regions  sum 
by  electrotonic  spread.  So  far,  only  the  over-all,  summed  potential 
has  been  observed  after  electrotonic  spread  along  the  axon  (the  so- 
called  synaptic  potential.^'  ''*'  ^2. 23,  25)  j^  setting  up  the  discharge  of 
impulses,  this  synaptic  potential  appears  to  act  just  as  a  catelectro- 
tonus,  the  discharge  occurring  at  a  critical  degree  of  depolarization. 
The  synaptic  potential  provides  a  satisfactory  explanation  of  all  the 
phenomena  hitherto  attributed  to  the  central  excitatory  state.-  22,25 
The  "detonator  response"^"'  ^^  need  no  longer  be  considered  as  a  sepa- 
rate entity,  for  that  hypothesis  was  based  on  experiments  now  explic- 
able, in  part,  by  the  flow  of  penetrating  current,  as  in  figure  5,  and,  in 
part,  by  the  postulated  local  response  of  the  post-synaptic  mem- 
brane.^^'  ^^'  ^^ 

Thus,  the  sequence  of  events  in  synaptic  transmission  is  envisaged 
as: 

(1)  Impulse  in  pre-synaptic  nerve  fiber  generates  a  current  which 
gives  a  diphasic  effect  at  the  synaptic  region  of  the  post-synaptic  cell, 
with  a  total  duration  of  probably  not  more  than  1  msec,  in  mammalian 
muscle  and  the  spinal  cord;  initial  anodal  focus,  with  cathodal  sur- 
round; more  intense  cathodal  focus,  with  anodal  surround. 

(2)  This  cathodal  focus  sets  up  a  brief  and  intense  local  response  at 
the  synaptic  region. 

(3)  From  this  local  response,  a  catelectrotonus  spreads  decrementally 
over  the  post-synaptic  cell  membrane. 

(4)  A  propagated  impulse  is  set  up  in  the  post-synaptic  cell,  if  this 
catelectrotonus  is  above  a  critical  value.  If  it  is  below,  then,  as  the 
local  response  subsides,  the  catelectrotonic  surround  decays  passively. 

6.     APPLICATIONS  OF  THE  HYPOTHESIS 

The  hypothesis  offers  an  explanation  of  the  following  observations 
on  synaptic  transmission: 

A.  Irreversibility  of  Synaptic  Transmission 

This  may  be  explained  in  the  following  three  ways:  (i)  An  impulse, 
artificially  set  up  in  the  post-synaptic  cell  and  fired  antidromically  at 


ECCLES:  ELECTRICAL  THEORIES  OF  TRANSMISSION         443 

the  synapse,  would,  in  general,  propagate  past  the  synapse  and  so  exert 
on  the  pre-synaptic  nerve  terminals  the  full  sequence  of  AiCiC2A2,  the 
terminal  A2  cutting  short  any  excitation  of  the  pre-synaptic  terminal 
by  CiCo;^  (iil  the  asymmetry  of  the  pre-  and  post-synaptic  elements, 
both  as  regards  relative  size  and  convergence  relationship;^  (iii)  the 
pre-synaptic  terminal  may  not  have  the  special  excitatory  properties 
postulated  for  the  synaptic  region  of  the  post-synaptic  cell  (assumption 
C).  Of  these,  (i)  is  susceptible  to  test  with  the  neuro-muscular  junc- 
tion. Antidromic  transmission  across  synapses  has  been  observed  only 
under  the  special  conditions  provided  by  the  prolonged  end-plate  nega- 
tivity set  up  by  nerw  impulses  in  eserinized  muscle.^'  There,  the  nerve 
terminal  would  be  sensitized  by  the  currents  generated  by  the  localized 
end-plate  negativity.*  Moreover,  some  of  the  muscle  impulses  may  be 
blocked  at  the  end-plate  by  the  catelectrotonus.^^-  ^^  Thus,  such  anti- 
dromic transmission  occurs  under  conditions  resembling  those  causing 
ephaptic  transmission,  close  to  a  killed  or  injured  region  of  nerve  (the 
Hering  effect,  part  3,  C,  iii).  The  present  hypothesis  would  predict 
that  antidromic  synaptic  transmission  would  be  greatly  facilitated  by 
colliding  two  nmscle  impulses  at  the  end-plate  region  (cf.  figure  4b). 
The  pre-synaptic  fiber  would  then  be  subjected  to  the  greatly  increased 
excitatory  action  of  double  strength  A1C2  stimulation,  and  antidromic 
transmission  sliould  occur  under  much  less  favorable  predisposing  con- 
ditions. 

B.  Synaptic  Delay 

If  the  initiation  of  the  post-synaptic  impulse  is  always  caused 
by  mediation  of  a  synaptic  potential  of  the  post-synaptic  cell,  then 
true  synaptic  delay  measures  the  interval  between  the  time  of  ar- 
rival at  the  synapse  of  the  fore-front  of  the  pre-synaptic  impulse 
and  the  initiation  of  the  synaptic  potential.  In  figure  7a,  the  time- 
course  of  the  action  potential  at  the  pre-synaptic  terminal  is  shown, 
and  below  it  (the  dotted  line  in  figure  7b),  the  first  derivative,  which 
gives  the  approximate  time-course  of  the  current  penetrating  the  post- 
synaptic membrane  (part  3,  C,  iii).  Allowance  for  rectification  action 
is  made  in  the  broken  line  of  figure  7b.  On  account  of  the  electric 
time  constant  of  this  membrane,  its  potential  change  (the  continuous 
line  in  figure  7b)  is  shown  lagging  behind  the  current  which  produces 
it.f  Now,  according  to  the  hypothesis,  the  post-synaptic  membrane 
initiates  a  local  response  when  the  catelectrotonus  reaches  a  critical 
value,  e.g.,  at  the  point  X,  in  figure  7c.    As  shown  in  figure  6d,  this 


*  Cf   Eccles,  J.  C,  &  J.  !■.  Malcolni.3"     Figure  15a, 
t  Cf.  Katz,  B.,  &  O.  H.  Schmitt."'     Figube  6. 


444 


ANNALS  NEW  YORK  ACADEMY  OF  SCIENCES 


(b) 


(c) 


(d) 


Figure  7.  (a)  Monophasic  pre-synaptic  action  potential,  (b)  Its  first  derivative  (dotted  line) 
giving  the  time-course  of  the  post-synapfic  penetrating  current  (part  3,  C),  anodal  currents  being 
plotted  downwards.  The  broken  line  shows  the  modification  produced  in  this  current  if  resistance 
is  doubled  at  the  anodal  focus  and  halved  at  the  cathodal  focus  (rectification).  The  continuous 
line  gives  the  approximate  time-course  of  the  post-synaptic  membrane  potential  so  produced,  al- 
lowance being  made  for  the  electric  time  constant  of  the  membrane  (cf.  Katz  &  Schmitf",  figure 
6).  (c)  Post-s>naptic  membrane  potential  shown  as  in  (b).  At  X,  the  local  response  of  the  syn- 
aptic membrane  is  initiated  by  the  catelectrotonic  phase,  and  it  is  shown  running  a  time-course 
rather  slower  than  a  spike,  (d)  Neglecting  the  passive  electrotonic  changes  of  (b)  and  (c),  this 
local  response  is  plotted  together  with  the  time-course  of  the  resulting  spreading  catelectrotonus, 
shown  as  the  broken  line  (the  synaptic  potential)  recorded  from  the  post-synaptic  membrane  adja- 
cent to  the  synapse  (e.f/.,  at  arrow,  in  figure  6d).  The  rising  phase  of  sin  initiated  impulse  is  also 
shown  (dotted  line),  AZ  being  its  synaptic  delay. 


ECCLES:  ELECTRICAL  THEORIES  OF  TRANSMISSION         445 

local  response  provides  the  active  region  for  setting  up  a  spreading 
catelectrotonus — the  synaptic  potential.  Thus,  the  synaptic  delay  (in 
FIGURE  7,  c  and  d),  is  represented  by  the  interval  AX.  The  value  of 
0.6  msec,  which  has  been  observed  for  this  synaptic  delay  at  mamma- 
lian neuro-muscular  junctions'*^'  ^*  and  motoneurone  synapses"^  (frog, 
1.3  msec.)  accords  well  with  tlie  duration  of  the  pre-synaptic  action 
potential  (figure  7a),  when  it  is  remembered  that  there  is  probably 
some  slowing  of  time-course,  as  the  impulse  propagates  into  the  fine 
pre-synaptic  terminals.'*®  As  shown  in  figure  7d,  a  further  delay,  XZ, 
usually  0.2  to  0.3  msec,  is  involved  in  the  building  up  of  the  synaptic 
potential  to  the  threshold,  for  initiating  an  impulse  at  Z."-  ^^'  ^^ 

On  the  basis  of  figure  7,  the  hypothesis  offers  a  satisfactory  explana- 
tion of  all  the  experimental  findings  on  synaptic  delay.  For  example: 
(i)  By  facilitation,  synaptic  delay  cannot  be  shortened  below  a  limit- 
ing value  of  about  0.5  msec,  for  central  synapses.^'-*  In  figure  7b  and 
7c,  AY  would  be  the  minimal  interval  at  which  excitation  could  occur, 
under  optimal  conditions  of  facilitation,  (ii)  Synaptic  delay  can, 
however,  be  further  shortened  by  the  direct  excitatory  action  of  a  pre- 
ceding subliminal  induction  shock. '"^  By  its  depolarizing  action,  the 
shock  would  diminish  the  time  lag  between  reversal  of  current  and  re- 
versal of  potential,  and  so  shorten  synaptic  delay  to  less  than  AY 
(figure  7b).  (iii)  The  longer  synaptic  delay  with  sympathetic  gang- 
lia (about  five  times  longer)  correlates  with  the  longer  duration  of  the 
pre-synaptic  spike.^"  which  sets  the  time  scale  throughout  figure  7. 
(iv)  The  upper  limiting  value  of  synaptic  delay,  for  example,  about 
1.0  to  1.5  msec,  for  mammalian  central  synapses'^'  "•  ^^'  ®°'  "  and  neuro- 
muscular junctions,-*''  ^^  has  been  correlated  with  the  time  of  the  rising 
phase  of  the  synaptic  potential,^'*'  ^'^  and  thus,  according  to  figure  7d, 
to  the  duration  of  the  local  response  of  the  post-synaptic  membrane 
(cf.  C,  below),  (v)  Synaptic  delay  (neuro-muscular  in  frog)"^  has, 
as  would  be  expected  from  figure  7,  approximately  the  same  tempera- 
ture coefficient  (2.1)  as  has  the  duration  of  the  spike  potential. 

C.  Time-Course  of  the  Active  Phase  of  the  Synaptic  Potential 

The  time-course  of  a  local  response  is  but  little  slower  than  the  spike 
potential.'*^'  ^-'  °^'  ^°'  ''^  According  to  the  hypothesis,  therefore,  the 
brief  phase  of  active  polarization  (determined  by  analysis  of  the  synap- 
tic potential)  should  have  a  time-course  somewhat  slower  than  the 
spike  of  the  post-synaptic  cell.  This  accords  well  with  the  findings  on 
ganglion  cells,^^  motoneurones,^^  and  muscles.^®  Furthermore,  the  tem- 
perature coefficient  of  this  "active  phase"  is  approximately  the  same 
as  for  a  spike.^^ 


446  ANNALS  NEW  YORK  ACADEMY  OF  SCIENCES 

D.  The  Brief  Period  of  Low  Resistance 

This  is  in  addition  to  that  attributable  to  catelectrotonus,  during  the 
initial,  "active"  phase  of  the  end-plate  potential.'*^  The  postulated 
local  response  of  the  end-plate  region  of  the  muscle  would  produce 
just  such  an  additional  fall  of  resistance,  running  the  same  time-course 
as  the  active  phase.* 

E.  Slow  Catelectrotonic  Potentials  in  the  Pre-Synaptic  Fibers^*  ^^ 

Such  potentials  have  only  been  observed  with  synapses  in  the  central 
nervous  system  (the  dorsal  root  potentials).  The  present  hypothesis 
has  been  extended  to  explain  the  production  of  these  potentials,  by 
making  the  additional  assumption  that  the  terminal  region  of  the  pre- 
synaptic fiber  resembles  the  post-synaptic  area,  in  being  specialized  to 
give  local  responses.  The  catelectrotonic  focus  provided  by  the  local 
response  of  the  post-synaptic  membrane  sets  up  the  current,  which  ex- 
cites the  pre-synaptic  terminal  to  give  a  local  response,  which,  in  turn, 
acts  as  a  focus,  setting  up  the  spreading  catelectrotonus  of  the  dorsal 
root  potential. ^° 

7.     DIFFICULTIES  OF  THE  HYPOTHESIS 

The  electrical  hypothesis  encounters  difficulties  in  explaining  the  fol- 
lowing experimental  observations,  but  possible  lines  of  reconciliation 
are  suggested. 

A.  Synaptic  Block  Produced  by  Curarine  in  Skeletal  Muscle 

and  Ganglia 

Curarine  acts  as  a  specific  depressant  of  the  excitatory  responses 
evoked  in  motor  end-plates  and  ganglion  cells  by  acetylcholine 
and  related  substances.^' '''  ^^'  "^'  ^^'  ^*  Thus,  the  acetylcholine  hypothe- 
sis provides  an  obvious  explanation  of  the  synaptic  blockage  produced 
by  curarine.  Now  curarine  causes  such  blockage  by  depressing  the 
synaptic  potential. ^^'  ^^'  ^^'  ^^  Hence,  according  to  the  electrical  hy- 
pothesis, the  simplest  explanation  of  the  blockage  would  be,  that  there 
is  depression  of  the  local  response  set  up  by  the  cathodal  focus  (cf. 
FIGURES  6d  and  7c) ;  i.e.,  that  curarine  depresses  the  electrical  excita- 
bility of  the  post-synaptic  membrane,  as  well  as  its  acetylcholine  ex- 
citability. In  a  recent  attempt  to  test  this,  by  electrical  stimulation 
of  the  motor  end-plate  in  the  isolated  nerve-muscle  fiber  preparation, 
before  and  after  curarization,  the  initiation  of  propagated  muscle  im- 


♦  Cf.  Katz,  B."     FiGtniB  11. 


ECCLES:  ELECTRICAL  THEORIES  OF  TRANSMISSION         447 

pulses  was  used  as  the  criterion  of  end-plate  excitability.  Yet,  if  the 
end-plate  region  reacts  by  local  responses,  rather  than  by  propagating 
impulses  (part  4,  C),  it  seems  probable  that  this  investigation  tested 
the  excitability,  not  of  the  end-plate  {i.e.,  of  the  post-synaptic  mem- 
brane), but  of  the  membrane  adjacent  thereto,  confirming  the  previ- 
ously observed  absence,  there,  of  curare  action. '^^'  '^-  '*  Thus,  it  pro- 
vides no  evidence  for,  or  against,  a  specific  depression  of  electrical  ex- 
citability of  the  post-synaptic  membrane.  Alternatively,  if  curarine 
blocks  solely  by  its  known  depressant  action  on  the  local  potentials  set 
up  by  acetylcholine,"'  ^^'  ^*  then,  in  very  deep  curarization,  the  re- 
sidual catelectrotonic  effects  produced  by  the  cathodal  focus,  i.e.,  by 
electrical  transmission,  should  be  observable,  uncomplicated  by  chem- 
ical transmission.  This  inference  is  particularly  pertinent  in  the  case 
of  sympathetic  ganglia,  where  the  synaptic  potential  is  virtually  abol- 
ished by  deep  curarization,  and  yet  other  evidence  suggests  that  acetyl- 
choline transmission  plays  but  a  minor  role  (part  2,  ii)  .^^ 

B.  Action  of  Anti-Cholinesterases  on  Synaptic  Transmission  with 
Skeletal  Muscle  and  Sympathetic  Ganglia 

Anti-cholinesterases  (eserine,  prostigmine)  delay  the  summit  of  the 
curarized  end-plate  potential  and  slow  its  decline,^^-  ^■*'  ^^  effects  which, 
undoubtedly,  are  attributable  to  a  prolongation  of  the  active  depolar- 
izing agent.  With  rapid,  repetitive  stimulation,  a  still  greater  effect  is 
observed,  the  end-plate  potential  persisting  for  several  seconds,  both  in 
curarized  and  normal  muscle."-  ^^  With  sympathetic  ganglia  (normal 
or  curarized),  anti-cholinesterases  also  cause  a  prolonged  synaptic  po- 
tential to  appear,  after  rapid,  repetitive  stimulation,  but  this  prolonged 
potential  is  sharply  distinguishable  as  a  special  addition  to  the  other- 
wise unaltered  synaptic  potential.-^  Presumably,  both  with  ganglia 
and  muscle,  the  prolonged  potential  is  due  to  acetylcholine  liberated  by 
pre-synaptic  impulses.*  With  ganglia,  it  has  been  argued  that,  since  the 
initial,  brief  transmitter  action,  setting  up  the  synaptic  potential,  is  un- 
affected by  anti-cholinesterases,  it  is  not  due  to  acetylcholine.^^  Simi- 
larly, with  muscle,  the  eserinized  (or  prostigminized)  end-plate  poten- 
tial appears  to  be  the  partly  fused  compound  of  a  brief,  initial  phase, 
but  little,  if  at  all,  lengthened  by  the  anti-cholinesterase  and  the  pro- 
longed phase  (certainly  due  to  acetylcholine)  .f  Thus,  the  electrical 
hypothesis  would  attribute  the  effect  of  anti-cholinesterases  to  an  in- 
tensification and  great  prolongation  of  the  normally  small  transmitter 
action  of  acetylcholine.     Incidentally,  it  may  be  noted  that  such  an 


•  However,  cf.  ITachmansolin,  D."* 

t  Eccles,  J.  C,  B.  Xatz,  8c  S.  W.  Kuffler.^"     Figure  5. 


448  ANNALS  NEW  YORK  ACADEMY  OF  SCIENCES 

acetylcholine  effect  would  account  well  for  the  latter  part  of  the  rela- 
tively long  transmitter  action  observed  at  normal  neuro-muscular  junc- 
tions (5  msec,  frog;^^  6  msec,  cat^^.)  A  similar  investigation  of  the 
synaptic  potentials  of  motoneurones  fails  to  reveal  any  such  effect  of 
anti-cholinesterases.  Hence,  it  would  appear  that  acetylcholine  plays  a 
negligible  role  at  such  synapses.  These  experiments  indicate  that  the 
synapses  of  nerve-muscle,  sympathetic  ganglia,  and  the  spinal  cord 
form  a  series  of  decreasing  significance  for  acetylcholine  transmission. 
There  has  been  no  reference  to  other  effects  of  anti-cholinesterases  on 
synaptic  transmission,  e.g.,  after-discharge,*'' "'  -^'  ^^  local  contrac- 
ture,^' ^•^'  ^^  lengthening  of  refractory  period, ^^'  ^^  or  catelectrotonic 
block,^'  ^^'  ^^'  2^  because  these  are  all  secondarily  produced  by  the  pro- 
longed and  intensified  synaptic  potential. 

C.  Repetitive  Synaptic  Transmission 
There  is  but  little  diminution  of  the  synaptic  potential  set  up  in 
curarized  mammalian  muscle^''  or  anesthetized  motoneurones^'^  by  a 
second  pre-synaptic  volley,  at  the  shortest  intervals  after  the  first. 
With  curarized  ganglia,  the  second  potential  is  usually  a  little  in- 
creased.-' The  great  increase  with  the  frog's  end-plate  potential^*''  ^*' 
^■^'  '^  raises  a  further,  as  yet  insoluble,  problem.  Since  local  responses 
are  followed  by  refractory  periods,*-'  ^"  assumption  C  of  the  hypothesis 
(part  4)  would  predict  a  considerable  diminution  of  a  second  synaptic 
potential,  at  short  intervals.  However,  it  must  be  remembered  that 
the  above  observations  relate  to  synaptic  potentials  diminished  suffi- 
ciently for  synaptic  blockage,  and  that  small  local  responses  set  up 
much  less  refractoriness  than  large  responses."'  '"  A  large  diminution 
of  synaptic  potential  is  observed,  when  synaptic  transmission  is  not 
blocked,-**'  -"'  ^'^  and  has,  hitherto,  been  attributed  to  the  refractoriness 
of  the  post-synaptic  cell.  Nevertheless,  it  may  be,  in  part,  due  to  re- 
fractoriness, following  the  postulated  large  local  responses  of  the  spe- 
cialized post-synaptic  membrane  (part  4,  C).  This  latter  explanation 
is  supported  by  the  observation  that  the  diminution  is  lessened  by  sub- 
paralytic  curarization.* 

8.     TESTING  THE  HYPOTHESIS 

It  has  been  shown  that  the  hypothesis  gives  a  satisfactory  explana- 
tion of  all  experiments  investigating  the  temporal  factors  in  synaptic 
transmission.  It  is  otherwise  with  the  intensity  factors.  Before  the 
hypothesis  can  be  regarded  as  well  established,  it  has  to  be  shown  that 

*  Bccles,  J.  C,  &  S.  W.  Xaffler'»:  505. 


ECCLES:  ELECTRICAL  THEORIES  OF  TRANSMISSION         449 

the  currents  generated  by  the  pre-synaptic  impulses  (figure  5)  excite 
the  post-synaptic  membrane  sufficiently  to  produce  the  observed  synap- 
tic potentials  and  initiation  of  impulses.  Such  an  intensity  of  action 
is  not  normally  attained,  even  by  the  most  efficient  ephapse/'  *^  but 
there  are  possibly  three  factors  increasing  the  efficiency  of  the  synapse: 

(i)  The  contact  of  the  pre-  and  post-synaptic  membranes  is  so  inti- 
mate^°  that  virtually  all  the  current  penetrating  the  former  must  pene- 
trate the  latter  (in  contrast  to  the  estimated  value  of  one  third  for  the 
ephapse*^) . 

(ii)  In  the  synapse,  a  large  expansion  in  the  area  of  contact  is  pro- 
duced by  the  swelling  and  branching  of  the  pre-synaptic  terminals; 
also,  with  nerve  cells,  by  the  multiplicity  of  closely  adjacent  synapses.^" 

(iii)  If  the  post-synaptic  membrane  has  special  excitatory  proper- 
ties (part  4,  C),  the  efficiency  of  electrical  excitation  may  be  thereby 
increased. 

By  stimulating  just  beyond  the  region  where  a  nerve  volley  is  blocked, 
Hodgkin"  (cf.  also  Lorente  de  N6'''°)  showed  that,  with  critical  condi- 
tions for  blockage,  the  threshold  may  be  lowered  to  only  10%  of  nor- 
mal, i.e.,  the  nerve  volley  still  provides  as  much  as  90%  of  the  threshold 
electrical  stimulus,  beyond  the  blockage.  Hence,  there  is  a  high  prob- 
ability that,  in  the  absence  of  block,  it  provides  an  electrical  stimulus 
adequate  to  excite,  i.e.,  that  the  transmission  of  nerve  impulses  is  elec- 
trical. It  should  be  possible  to  apply  a  similar  test  to  the  curarized 
end-plate  of  the  isolated  nerve-muscle  fiber  preparation.  It  has,  of 
course,  been  shown  that  there  is  a  lowering  of  threshold,  during  the 
end-plate  potential,*^-  ^^  as  would  be  expected  for  a  catelectrotonus, 
however  produced.  The  present  test  would  explore,  instead,  the  brief 
interval  of  pre-synaptic  current  flow,  particularly  that  preceding  the 
origin  of  the  synaptic  potential  {i.e.,  AX  in  figure  7C).  Such  a  test 
has  already  given  suggestive  results  with  motoneurones,^*  but,  in  order 
to  be  convincing,  there  should  be  an  accurate  location  of  the  stimulat- 
ing electrode  on  the  synaptic  region  of  the  post-synaptic  membrane, 
and,  at  present,  this  seems  possible  only  with  the  isolated  nerve- 
muscle  fiber  (cf.  Kufiler^*).  An  attempt  on  the  whole  sartorius  was 
unsuccessful.* 

Crucial  testing  of  the  hypothesis  will  also  be  provided  by  further 
pharmacological  experiments  on  synaptic  transmission:  particularly 
the  action  of  anti-cholinesterases,  and  the  effects  of  various  background 
concentrations  of  acetylcholine,  potassium,  and  calcium.  Predictions 
leading  to  tests  have  already  been  mentioned  in  the  preceding  sections. 


♦  Katz,  B.«:  K9t;. 


450  ANNALS  NEW  YORK  ACADKMY  OF  SCIENCES 

111  addition,  iiiHtlifiiiatical  treatment  of  the  flow  of  penetrating  current 
at  a  schematized  synapse  should  be  possible,  and  would  give  more  pre- 
cise predictions  for  experimental  testing.  Since  the  hypothesis  is 
based  on  the  investigations  on  nerve  and  muscle  fibers  outlined  in 
PART  3  (particularly  the  ephaptic  experimentsj ,  further  developments 
of  this  work  are  of  immediate  relevance  as  tests  of  the  hypothesis, 
providing  data  on  which  will  depend  its  development,  or  modification, 
or  rejection  in  whole  or  in  part. 

Finally,  it  may  be  stated  that  a  recommendation  for  the  hypothesis 
is  its  systematization  of  synapses  and  neuro-muscular  junctions  in  a 
series  (neuro-muscular  junctions,  ganglionic  synapses,  and  central 
synapses) ,  exhibiting  a  progressive  replacement  of  acetylcholine  trans- 
mission by  electrical  transmission.  To  the  beginning  of  such  a  series 
could  be  added  those  special  modifications  of  ganglionic  and  neuro- 
muscular synapses,  seen,  respectively,  with  the  chromaffin  organs  (su- 
prarenal medulla)  and  electric  organs,  where  synaptic  transmission 
seems  to  be  wholly  due  to  acetylcholine. 

9.     SUMMARY 

•  Transmission  of  impulses  across  synapses  of  the  spinal  cord,  sympa- 
thetic ganglia,  and  skeletal  muscle,  involves  a  dual  problem:  (a)  the 
setting  up  of  synaptic,  catelectrotonic  potentials  in  the  post-synaptic 
cell;  and  (b)  the  initiation  of  impulses  in  the  post-synaptic  cell  by  such 
potentials.  Evidence  is  given  that,  in  their  present  form,  both  the 
chemical  (acetylcholine)  and  electrical  hypotheses  relating  to  Prob- 
lem (a)  are  unsatisfactory.  Furthermore,  recent  experiments  are 
cited  which  indicate  that  acetylcholine  plays  a  negligible  part  as  a 
synaptic  transmitter  with  motoneurones ;  a  subsidiary  role  with  sym- 
pathetic ganglia,  and  possibly  also  with  skeletal  muscle.  Hence,  it  is 
desirable  to  attempt  a  more  precise  formulation  of  the  electrical  hy- 
pothesis. 

.  The  present  attempt  is  based,  mainly,  on  four  lines  of  recent  inves- 
tigation : 

(1)  The  electrical  properties  of  surface  membranes:  resistance,  elec- 
tromotive force,  capacity,  and  rectification.  Inductance  is  neglected, 
because  it  is  doubtful  if  the  high  values  of  cephalopod  axons  obtain  for 
vertebrate  nerve  and  muscle. 

(2)  The  existence  of  active  load  responses.  These  may  be  very 
large  in  refractory,  or  anesthetized,  or  deteriorated  nerve. 

(3)  The  electrical  actions  occnrring  (uto.s.s  artificial  synapses  [ephap- 
ses) .     There  are  shown  to  be  three  main  types  of  ejih apses,  the  synapse 


ECCLES:  ELECTRICAL  THEORIES  OF  TRANSMISSION  451 

being  a  special  example  of  the  type  with  an  initial  anodal  and  terminal 
cathodal  action;  the  only  type  in  which  a  significant  excitatory  action 
is  exerted. 

1.4)  Electrical  recording  from  the  isolated  neuro-muscular  junction 
shows  that  the  motor  end-plate  is  specialized  to  give  local  responses 
without  the  all-or-nothing  breakdown  of  propagated  impulses.  Im- 
pulse initiation  appears  to  be  produced  by  a  secondary  catelectrotonus 
in  the  surrounding  membrane. 

The  present  hypothesis  makes  three  main  assumptions: 

A.  A  schematized  formulation  is  made  of  the  essential  geometrical 
relationship  of  the  membranes  of  the  pre-  and  post-synaptic  elements, 
as  revealed  by  histological  and  electrical  investigation. 

B.  The  electrical  properties  of  the  pre-  and  post-synaptic  surface 
membranes  resemble  those  observed  for  peripheral  nerve  and  muscle 
(see  (.1),  above j. 

C.  The  membrane  of  the  immediate  post-synaptic  region  is  special- 
ized, so  that  large  and  graduated  local  responses  are  set  up  by  catelec- 
trotonic  polarization  (see  (2)  and  (4),  above). 

On  these  basic  assumptions,  it  is  shown  that  a  pre-synaptic  im- 
pulse sets  up  electric  currents  exerting  an  initial  anodal  and  later 
cathodal  action  on  the  post-synaptic  membrane.  The  latter  action,  in- 
tensified by  rectification,  sets  up  a  local  response  (part  4,  C),  which,  in 
turn,  acts  as  a  relatively  prolonged  cathodal  focus,  from  which  spreads, 
electrotonically,  the  synaptic  potential  of  the  effector  cell.  Finally, 
the  initiation  of  impulses  by  this  synaptic  potential  appears  to  be  ex- 
plicable, simply,  as  the  action  of  a  catelectrotonus. 

This  hypothesis  is  shown  to  offer  satisfactory  explanations  of  many 
fundamental  observations  on  synaptic  transmission:  irreversibility; 
synaptic  delay;  time-course  of  junctional  potential;  brief  impedance 
loss  at  end-plates;  dorsal  root  potentials  of  the  spinal  cord;  some  of 
which  were  hitherto  inexplicable  in  detail. 

On  the  other  hand,  the  hypothesis  encounters  difficulties  in  explain- 
ing the  actions  of  curare  and  of  anti-cholinesterases  on  synaptic  trans- 
mission in  ganglia  and  skeletal  muscle.  The  action  of  curare  may  be 
explained,  if  it  is  assumed  that  it  depresses  the  electrical  excitability  of 
the  post-synaptic  membrane,  as  well  as  its  pharmacological  excitabil- 
ity. It  is  argued  that  this  assumption  has  not  yet  been  tested.  The 
action  of  anti-cholinesterases  is  attributed  to  the  intensification  and 
prolongation  of  the  action  of  acetylcholine,  to  which  the  hypothesis  as- 
cribes a  subsidiary  role,  as  a  transmitter  at  synapses  of  ganglia  and 
skeletal  muscle.     A  further  difficulty  appears  to  arise  in  the  explana- 


452  ANNALS  NEW  YORK  ACADEMY  OF  SCIENCES 

tion  of  rapid,  repetitive,  synaptic  transmission.  The  postulated  local 
responses  should  be  followed  by  refractory  periods,  but  a  possible  ex- 
planation is  suggested. 

The  testing  of  the  hypothesis  is  shown,  especially,  to  concern  the 
further  investigation  of  the  special  electrical  properties  assumed  for 
the  post-synaptic  membrane ;  also,  the  attempt  to  discover  how  far  the 
postulated  electrical  actions  can  account  quantitatively  for  the  post- 
synaptic stimulation.  It  has  been  shown  (above)  that  the  observed 
temporal  course  of  the  post-synaptic  stimulation  is  satisfactorily  ex- 
plained. In  addition,  further  pharmacological  investigation  is  neces- 
sary to  test  the  explanation  attributing  a  subsidiary  role  to  acetyl- 
choline transmission.  It  is  evident  that  further  work  on  the  electrical 
properties  of  membranes  on  local  responses,  and  on  ephaptic  transmis- 
sion, will  provide  additional  tests  of  the  hypothesis. 

BIBLIOGRAPHY 

1.  Arvanitaki,  A. 

1942.     J.  Neurophysiol.  5:  89. 

2.  Barron,  D.  H.,  &  B.  H.  C.  Matthews 

1938.  J.  Physiol.  92:276. 

3.  Bishop,  G.  H. 

1941.  Ann.  Rev.  Physiol.  3:  1. 

3a.  Bremer,  F.,  V.  Bonnet,  &  J.  Moldaver 

1942.  J.  Arch.  Int.  Physiol.  52:  1. 

4.  Bronk,  D.  W. 

1939.  J.  Neurophysiol.  2:  380. 

5.  Brown,  G.  L. 

1937.     J.  Physiol.  89:438. 

6.  Brown,  G.  L. 

1937.  Physiol.  Rev.  17:  485. 

7.  Brown,  G.  L.,  H.  H.  Dale,  &  W.  Feldberg 

1936.     J.  Physiol.  87:394. 

8.  Buchthal,  F.,  &  J.  Lindhard 

1939.     'i'lie  Physiology  of  Striated  Muscle  Fiber.     Munksgaard.  Copenhagen. 

9.  Buchthal,  F.,  &  J.  Lindhard 
1942.     Act.  Physiol.  Skand.  4:  136. 

10.  Cajal,  S.  R. 

1934.     Trav.  Lab.  Rech.  Biol.  Madrid  29:  1. 

11.  Clark,  A.  J. 

1936.     Proc.  Roy.  Soc.  London  B  121 :  580. 

12.  Cole,  K.  S. 

1941.     J.  Gen.  Physiol.  25:  29. 

13.  Cole,  K.  S.,  &  R.  F.  Baker 
1941.     J.  Gen.  Physiol.  24:  535. 

14.  Cole,  K.  S.,  &  R.  F.  Baker 
1941.     J.  Gen.  Physiol.  24:  771. 

15.  Cole,  K.  S.,  &  H.  J.  Curtis 

1938.  J.  Gen.  Physiol.  22:  37. 

16.  Cole,  K.  S.,  &  H.  J.  Curtis 

1939.  J.  Gen.  Physiol.  22:649. 


ECCLES:  ELECTRICAL  THEORIES  OF  TRANSMISSION         453 

17.  Cole,  K.  S.,  &  H.  J.  Curtis 

1941.     J.  Gen.  Physiol.  24:  551. 

18.  Cowan,  S.  L. 

1936.  J.  Physiol.  88:  30. 

19.  Dale,  H.  H. 

1937.  Harvey  Lectures  32 :  229. 

20.  Eccles,  J.  C. 

1936.  Ergebn.  Physiol.  38:  339. 

21.  Eccles,  J.  C. 

1939.     Ann.  Rev.  Physiol.  1:  363. 

22.  Eccles,  J.  C. 

1943.  J.  Physiol.  101:465. 

23.  Eccles,  J.  C. 

1944.  J.  Physiol.  103:27. 

24.  Eccles,  J.  C. 

1945.  Nature  156:  680. 

25.  Eccles,  J.  C. 

1946.  J.  Neurophysiol.  9:  87 

26.  Eccles,  J.  C,  B.  Katz,  &  S.  W.  Kuffler 

1941.  J.  Neurophysiol.  4:  362. 

27.  Eccles,  J.  C,  B.  Katz,  &  S.  W.  Kuffler 

1942.  J.  Neurophysiol.  5:  221,  225,  227-228. 

28.  Eccles,  J.  C,  «fe  S.  W.  Kuffler 
1941.     J.  Neurophysiol.  4:  402. 

29.  Eccles,  J.  C,  &  S.  W.  Kuffler 
1941.     J.  Neurophysiol.  4:  486. 

30.  Eccles,  J.  C,  &  J.  L.  Malcolm 
Sent  in  to  J.  Neurophysiol. 

31.  Eccles,  J.  C,  &  J.  L.  Malcolm 
Unpublished  Observations. 

32.  Erlanger,  J. 

1939.  J.  Neurophysiol.  2:  370. 

33.  Feng,  T.  P. 

1937.  Chin.  J.  Physiol.  12:  331. 

34.  Feng,  T.  P. 

1940.  Chin.  J.  Physiol.  15:367. 

35.  Feng,  T.  P. 

1941.  Biol.  Symp.  3:  121. 

36.  Fillenz,  M.,  &  M.  Hanafin 
Unpublished  Observations. 

37.  Gerard,  R.  W. 

1942.  Ann.  Rev.  Physiol.  4:  329. 

38.  Granit,  R.,  &  C.  R.  Skoglund 
1945.     J.  Physiol.  103:435. 

39.  Grundfest,  H. 

1932.     J.  Physiol.  76:  95. 

40.  Grundfest,  H. 

1939.     Am.  J.  Physiol.  127:  252. 

41.  Hodgkin,  A.  L. 

1937.  J.  Physiol.  90:  183. 

42.  Hodgkin,  A.  L. 

1938.  Proc.  Roy.  Soc.  London  B  126:  87. 

43.  Jasper,  H.  H.,  &  A.  M.  Monnier 
1938.    J.  Cell.  Comp.  Physiol.  11:  259. 

44.  Katz,  B. 

1937.     Proc.  Roy.  Soc.  London  B  124:  244. 


454  ANNALS  NEW  YORK  ACADEMY  OF  SCIENCES 

45.  Katz,  B. 

1939.     J.  Physiol.  95:  286. 

46.  Katz,  B. 

1939.     Electric  Excitation  of  Nerve.     Oxford  Univ.  Press. 

47.  Katz,  B. 

1942.     J.  Neurophysiol.  5:  169. 

48.  Katz,  B.,  &  O.  H.  Schmitt 

1939.     J.  Phy.siol.  97:  471. 

49.  Katz,  B.,  &  O.  H.  Schmitt 

1942.     J.  Physiol.  100:  369. 

50.  Kuffler,  S.  W. 

1942.     J.  Neurophy.siol.  5:  18. 

51.  Kuffler,  S.  W. 

1942.     J.  Neurophysiol.  5:  199. 

52.  Kuffler,  S.  W. 

1942.  J.  Neurophysiol.  5:  309. 

53.  Kuffler,  S.  W. 

1943.  J.  Neurophysiol.  6:  99. 

54.  Kuffler,  S.  W. 

1945.     J.  Neurophysiol.  8:  77. 

66.  Langley,  J.  N. 

1907.     J.  Physiol.  36:  347. 

56.  Lloyd,  D.  P.  C. 

1943.    J.  Neurophysiol.  6:  143. 

57.  Lloyd,  D.  P.  C. 

1943.  J.  Neurophysiol.  6:  293. 

58.  Lorente  de  No,  R. 

1935.  J.  Cell.  Comp.  Physiol.  7:  47. 

59.  Lorente  de  No,  R. 

1938.  J.  Neurophysiol.  1:  187. 

60.  Lorente  de  No,  R. 

1939.  J.  Neurophysiol.  2:  402. 

61.  Lorente  de  No,  R. 

1944.  J.  Cell.  Comp.  Physiol.  24:  85. 

62.  Macintosh,  F.  C. 

1941.     J.  Physiol.  99:436. 

63.  Marmont,  G. 

1941.  J.  Physiol.  133:  376P. 

64.  Marrazzi,  A.  S.,  &  R.  Lorente  de  No 

1944.  J.  Neurophysiol.  7:  83 

66.  Monnier,  A.  M. 

1934.     L'e.xcitation  61ectrique  des  tissus.     Hermann.     Paris 

66.  Monnier,  A.  M. 

1936.  Cold  Spr.  Harb.  Symp.  4:111. 

67.  Nachmansohn,  D. 

1940.  Yale  J.  Biol.  Med.  12:  565. 

68.  Nachmansohn,  D. 

1945.  Vitamins  and  Hormones  3:  337. 

69.  Osterhout,  W.  J.  V.,  &  S.  E.  Hill 

1930.     J.  G3n.  Physiol.  13:  547. 

70.  Pumphrey,  R.  J.,  O.  H.  Schmitt,  &  J.  Z.  Young 
1940.     J.  Physiol.  98:  47. 

71.  Renshaw,  B. 

1940.     J.  Neurophysiol.  3:  373. 

72.  Renshaw,  B. 

1942.  J.  Neurophysiol.  5:  23.'^ 


ECCLES:  ELECTRICAL  THEORIES  OF  TRANSMISSION         455 

73.  Renshaw,  B.,  &  P.  O.  Therman 

1941.     Am.  J.  Physiol.  133:  98. 

74.  Rosenblueth,  A. 

1911.     Am.  J.  Physiol.  132:  119. 

75.  Rushton,  W.  A.  H. 
19.33.     J.  Physiol.  77:  337. 

76.  Rushton,  W.  A.  H. 

1937.  Proc.  Roy.  Soo.  London  B  124:  201. 

77.  Schaefer,  H.,  &  P.  Haass 

1939.  PHug.  Arch.  ges.  Physiol.  242 :  364. 

78.  Schaefer,  H.,  P.  Scholmerich,  &  P.  Haass 

1938.  PHug.  Arch.  ges.  Physiol.  241:  310. 

79.  Schmitt,  F.  O.,  &  O.  H.  Schmitt 

1940.  J.  Physiol.  98:  26. 


CHEMICAL  EXCITATION  OF  NERVE* 

By  Frank  Brink,  jR.,t  Detlev  W.  Bronk,  and  Martin  G.  Larrabee 

Eldridge  Reeves  Johnson  Research  Foundation,  University  of  Pennsylvania, 

Philadelphia,  Pennsylvania 

One  of  the  noteworthy  characteristics  of  neurones  is  their  sensitivity 
to  changes  in  the  chemical  environment.  Even  within  the  relatively 
protected  interior  of  the  body,  the  properties  of  nerves  are  subject  to 
modification  by  variations  in  the  composition  of  the  body  fluids.  In- 
deed, the  alterations  of  irritability  and  the  trains  of  nerve  impulses, 
which  are  the  result  of  changes  in  the  chemical  environment,  are  among 
the  most  important  factors  involved  in  the  regulation  of  the  activity 
of  the  organism.  This  is  one  of  the  significant  reasons  for  studying 
the  chemical  activation  of  nerve.  A  second  reason  derives  from  the 
current  interest  in  the  role  of  chemical  agents  in  the  mechanism  of  syn- 
aptic transmission.  Furthermore,  the  investigation  of  the  effects  of 
various  chemical  agents  is  one  of  the  most  fruitful  sources  of  informa- 
tion regarding  the  role  of  the  several  chemical  components  of  the  nerve 
structure  and  of  the  chemical  processes  involved  in  nervous  action. 

This  last  consideration  suggests  that  the  most  significant  chemical 
agents  for  use  in  the  study  of  the  processes  of  activation  are  those 
which  have  an  important  part  in  the  normal  structure  of  nerve.  Cal- 
cium is  such  an  element.  Potassium  is  another;  it  modifies  the  action 
of  calcium,  to  which  it  is  closely  related  in  the  regulation  of  nerve  ac- 
tion, and  it  has  a  marked  influence  on  the  electric  potential  difference 
across  the  interfaces  at  which  the  nerve  comes  in  contact  with  its  en- 
vironment. Finally,  the  effects  of  acetylcholine  on  the  initiation  and 
conduction  of  the  nerve  impulse  make  an  important  and  timely 
subject  for  investigation  in  such  a  study  as  this.  It  is  with  the  effects 
of  these  agents  that  we  shall  be  primarily  concerned.  There  are 
others  of  significance  for  a  general  study  of  this  problem,  but  from 
these  three  we  can  derive  many  of  the  basic  phenomena  involved  in 
chemical  excitation. 


The  changes  in  the  functional  characteristics  of  a  nerve  caused  by 
an  alteration  of  its  chemical  environment  are  due  to  the  consequent 


*  The  experimental  work  reported  here  has  been  generously  supported  by  grants  from  the  Supreme 
Council,  Scottish  Rite  Masons,  and  from  the  American  Philosophical  Society, 
t  Fellow  of  the  Lalor  Foundation. 

(457)  


458  ANNALS  NEW  YORK  ACADEMY  OF  SCIENCES 

changes  in  the  chemical  constitution  of  the  cell  or  axon.  Thus,  the  in- 
creased irritability  that  is  induced  by  surrounding  a  nerve  with  a  cal- 
cium-deficient fluid  follows  a  decrease  of  calcium  within  the  nerve. 
In  order  that  the  effects  of  such  changes  may  be  investigated,  it  is  con- 
venient to  have  available  a  solution  which  will  maintain  nerve  in  a 
stable  functional  state  for  long  periods  of  time,  and  to  which  the  effects 
of  other  environmental  solutions  may  be  referred.  It  is  customary  to 
choose  for  reference  a  solution  having  a  salt  content  and  pH  approxi- 
mating that  of  the  animal's  body  fluids.  By  direct  test,  it  has  been 
found  that  a  frog  nerve  can  be  kept  in  a  solution  at  pH  7.2  (phosphate 
buffer)  containing  sodium  chloride  (116  mM),  potassium  chloride  (2.0 
mM),  and  calcium  chloride  (1.8  mM)  for  many  hours,  with  no  sig- 
nificant change  in  excitability  or  in  rate  of  aerobic  oxidation.  Squid 
nerve,  which  we  have  also  employed,  maintains  a  similar  stable  func- 
tional state  in  sea  water  (Woods  Hole)  at  pH  8.0  or  in  a  solution  con- 
taining sodium  chloride  (405  mM),  potassium  chloride  (11  mM),  and 
calcium  chloride  (70  mM).  Modifications  of  these  solutions  have  been 
used  as  the  experimental  means  of  chemical  activation. 

The  calcium  ion  concentration  of  the  environmental  fluid  is  espe- 
cially important  in  determining  the  excitability  of  nerve.  This  familiar 
phenomenon  (cf.,  e.g.,  MisskeM  can  be  studied  quantitatively  and  under 
quickly  reversible  conditions  in  squid  giant  axons  or  in  bundles  of  frog 
axons  from  which  the  perineurium  has  been  removed.  Under  those  cir- 
cumstances, diffusion  equilibrium  between  the  axons  and  the  surround- 
ing fluid  is  attained  relatively  quickly.  In  figure  1,  the  threshold 
strength  of  direct  current  necessary  to  initiate  an  impulse,  which  is  the 
rheobase,  is  plotted  as  a  function  of  the  concentration  of  calcium 
chloride  in  the  fluid  bathing  a  giant  axon  of  the  squid.  A  similar  re- 
lation is  obtained  for  the  a  fibers  in  a  frog  sciatic  nerve  (figure  2). 

The  increased  excitability  produced  by  the  action  of  solutions  having 
a  low  concentration  of  calcium  chloride  is  presumably  due  to  the  dif- 
fusion of  Ca^+  from  the  cell  structure.  Indeed,  Tipton'  has  shown  by 
chemical  analysis  that  as  much  as  40  per  cent  of  the  total  calcium  of 
frog  nerve  is  in  diffusion  equilibrium  with  the  surrounding  fluid,  some 
of  this  diffusible  calcium  being  in  the  cell  phase.  His  evidence  for 
intracellular  precipitation  of  added  calcium  is  a  further  indication  that 
changes  in  the  calcium  chloride  content  of  the  bathing  fluids  lead  to 
changes  in  the  cellular  content.  The  spatial  distribution  of  these 
changes  in  cellular  calcium  are  unknown. 

When  frog  nerve  is  equilibrated  with  solutions  containing  from  1.0 
mM  to  0.3  mM  calcium  chloride,  or  when  squid  nerve  is  equilibrated 


BRINK  AND  OTHERS:  CHEMICAL  EXCITATION  OF  NERVE    459 
100 


o 

X 
CO 


FiGTJRB  1.  Threshold  of  giant  axon  of  Squid  in  isotonic  solutions  containing  various  concentra- 
tions of  CaCh.  Threshold  (rheobase)  as  per  cent  of  threshold  of  nerve  in  Woods  Hole  sea 
water.  Concentration  in  millLmoles  per  liter.  The  arrow  indicates  concentration  at  which  spon- 
taneous activity  began. 


FiGDEE  2.  Threshold  of  a  fibers  of  sciatic  nerve  of  frog  with  various  concentrations  (millimoles 
per  liter)  of  CaCh  in  bathing  fluid.  Threshold  as  per  cent  of  rheobase  of  nerve  m  reference 
aolution  described  in  text. 


460  ANNALS  NEW  YORK  ACADEMY  OF  SCIENCES 

with  solutions  containing  from  about  70  mM  to  10  mM  calcium  chlo- 
ride, there  is  a  certain  degree  of  irritability  corresponding  to  each  con- 
centration. This  is  measured  as  the  minimal  strength  of  current  nec- 
essary to  initiate  a  conducted  impulse.  It  may  be  thought  of  as  an 
index  of  the  stability  of  the  excitable  portion  of  the  nerve  structure. 
We  shall  subsequently  refer  to  the  fact  that  the  rate  of  oxygen  con- 
sumption of  nerve  is  also  modified  by  changes  in  its  calcium  content. 
Here,  it  is  pertinent  to  remark  that  the  variations  of  oxygen  consump- 
tion occur  within  this  same  range  of  calcium  concentrations  in  which 
there  are  measurable  changes  of  irritability.  In  the  case  of  frog  nerve, 
moderate  increases  in  calcium  above  2.0  mM  do  not  cause  a  further 
appreciable  decrease  of  irritability  nor  a  further  decrease  of  oxygen 
consumption.  At  very  much  higher  concentrations,  above  15  mM, 
the  irritability  again  decreases,^  and  there  is  a  further  fall  in  the  oxygen 
consumption.*  If  the  concentration  of  calcium  be  lowered  beyond  0.3 
mM,  or  10  mM  in  the  case  of  squid  nerve,  it  is  no  longer  possible  to 
measure  the  irritability  in  terms  of  the  strength  of  current  necessary  to 
initiate  an  impulse.  At  these  levels,  the  nerve  structure  has  been  so 
much  modified,  its  stability  lowered  so  much,  that  it  goes  through 
periodically-recurring  cycles  of  change,  with  consequent,  self-initiated 
trains  of  propagated  impulses.^ 

II 

The  response  of  nerve  to  the  exciting  action  of  an  electric  current 
can  be  studied  in  a  nerve  trunk  or  in  a  bundle  of  fibers.  The  stimulus 
is  under  the  control  of  the  experimenter,  and  all  of  the  fibers  are  ex- 
cited simultaneously.  Accordingly,  the  action  potential  recorded  from 
the  aggregate  of  fibers  of  a  given  type  is  a  fairly  accurate  representa- 
tion of  the  sequence  of  events  in  each  fiber,  provided  temporal  disper- 
sion, due  to  differences  in  conduction  velocity,  is  avoided. 

The  situation  is  quite  different  in  the  case  of  chemical  excitation. 
The  altered  chemical  environment  modifies  the  properties  of  the  fibers, 
so  that  a  sequence  of  cychc  events  develops  in  each  fiber,  with  a  fre- 
quency that  is  determined  by  the  characteristics  of  the  fiber.  Because 
these  intrinsic  characteristics  differ,  the  frequency  of  the  impulses  dis- 
charged from  a  chemically  treated  region  varies  from  fiber  to  fiber. 
Furthermore,  the  properties  of  the  fiber  may  change  from  moment  to 
moment,  so  that  the  sequence  of  impulses  is  not  truly  periodic.  Finally, 
the  times  of  initiation  of  impulses  in  one  fiber  are  independent  of  the 
timing  of  these  events  in  the  other  fibers,  in  contrast  to  the  externally 
determined  synchronization  imposed  by  electric  stimuli.     Because  of 


BRINK  AND  OTHERS:  CHEMICAL  EXCITATION  OF  NERVE    461 

these  considerations,  the  action  potentials  recorded  from  a  bundle  of 
numerous  fibers  reveal  little  of  what  is  occurring  in  the  individual  units 
(figure  3).  Under  these  conditions,  the  investigation  of  the  proc- 
esses of  excitation  and  response  encounters  the  same  difficulties  experi- 
enced in  the  study  of  groups  of  sensory  endings  or  motor  nerve  cells 
(Adrian  f  Adrian  and  Zotterman  ;^  Adrian  and  Bronk*) . 

The  solution  here  is  the  same  as  there :  that  is,  to  isolate  and  measure 
the  activity  in  a  single  fiber.  Only  when  this  is  done  can  one  observe 
the  more  or  less  rhythmic  train  of  impulses  discharged  from  the  chem- 
ically modified  region    (figure  3).     The  difficulties  inherent  in  this 


i 


Figure  3.  Above:  Impulses  recorded  from  branch  of  sciatic  nerve  of  frog  stimulated  by  topical  ap- 
plication of  isotonic  sodium  citrate.  Below:  Impulses  recorded  from  single  «  fiber  dissected  from 
this  nerve.     Time  in  1/5  seconds. 

experimental  procedure  partly  explain  the  relative  paucity  of  our 
knowledge  regarding  the  nature  of  chemical  excitation. 

It  is  worthy  of  emphasis  that  an  axon  possesses  the  capacity  (as  does 
a  cell  body  or  sensory  ending)  for  transforming  the  continuous  environ- 
mental action  of  a  physical  or  chemical  agent  into  a  series  of  recurrine 
events  which  are  made  manifest  as  nerve  impulses. 

It  has  been  said  that  the  frequency  of  impulses  developed  varies  from 
fiber  to  fiber,  and  depends  upon  the  intrinsic  characteristics  of  each 


462 


ANNALS  NEW  YORK  ACADEMY  OF  SCIENCES 


fiber.  Obviously,  these  properties  will  be  modified  by  the  chemical 
excitant,  qualitatively  by  the  nature  of  the  chemical  change,  and  to  a 
degree  that  is  determined  by  the  amount  of  substance  added  to,  or  re- 
moved from,  the  nerve  structure.  Therefore,  it  is  not  surprising  to 
find  that  the  number  of  impulses  discharged  per  second  from  the  treated 
region  depends  upon  the  concentration  of  the  calcium  ion,  as  shown  in 
FIGURE  4. 


Figure  4.  The  average  frequency  of  impulse;^  recorded  from  a  single  fiber  depends  UDon  the 
concentration  of  calcium  ions.  Upper  record,  concentration  of  Ca*  is  0.3  mM ;  middle,  0.1  mM ; 
lower,  no  calcium.  In  this  experiment,  the  0.1  mM  calcium  was  applied  first,  then  tiie  0.3  mM, 
and  finally  the  calcium-free  solution.    Time  in  1/5  seconds. 

The  frequency  of  impulses  initiated  by  a  given  reduction  of  calcium 
ion  concentration,  or  by  other  chemically  stimulating  media,  also  de- 
pends upon  the  previous  duration  of  the  chemical  action.  The  electric 
threshold  begins  to  fall  almost  at  once  after  the  application  of  the 
solution  (figure  5).  Further  time  is  required  for  changes  in  the  intra- 
cellular processes  which  must  precede  the  development  of  conducted 
impulses.  Indeed,  the  first  impulse  may  not  develop  for  some  minutes, 
and  then  at  a  time  when  the  threshold  has  fallen  to  zero.  This  same 
gradual  loss  of  stability,  continuing  further,  is  manifest  in  the  pro- 
gressive increase  in  the  average  frequency  of  impulses. 

When  calcium  is  removed  from  a  nerve,  by  diff'usion  into  a  solution 
containing  less  than  the  normal  amount  of  calcium  chloride,  the  dis- 


BRINK  AND  OTHERS:  CHEMICAL  EXCITATION  OF  NERVE    463 

charge  of  impulses  referred  to  above  begins  slowly.  If  the  calcium 
ion  concentration  is  reduced,  by  adding  a  calcium-binding  agent  such 
as  sodium  citrate,  the  discharge  begins  more  quickly  and  continues 
longer.  Because  of  this,  we  have  used  for  many  of  our  experiments  a 
stimulating  fluid  which  contained  sodium  citrate  in  place  of  some  of 
the  sodium  chloride.  When  the  calcium  ion  concentration  is  thus  re- 
duced below  about  0.4  mM,  impulses  are  initiated.     This  is  the  concen- 


FiGUHE  5.  The  threshold  of  an  axon  decreases  with  time  after  topical  application  (upper  arrow) 
of  a  solution  which  lowers  the  calcium  ion  concentration.  Repetitive  activity  begins  at  14  min. 
after  rheobase  has  decreased  below  five  per  cent  of  its  initial  value.  Threshold  measured  with 
cathode  on  calcium-deficient  region  of  nerve. 

tration  level  for  activation,  whether  the  calcium  be  removed  by  the 
action  of  citrate  or  by  the  simple  process  of  diffusion.  This,  and  other 
evidence,  suggest  that  a  principal  factor  in  citrate  excitation  is  the  low- 
ered calcium  ion  concentration. 

We  have  already  stated  that  the  frequency  of  impulses  initiated  by 
the  removal  of  calcium  is,  at  any  time,  dependent  upon  the  duration 
of  the  previous  action  of  the  stimulating  fluid.  The  time-course  of 
development  of  the  impulse  discharge  is  also  largely  influenced  by  the 
previous  chemical  treatment  of  the  nerve.  Usually,  the  impulses  be- 
gin to  occur,  at  random  intervals,  when  the  rheobase  has  fallen  to  about 
5  per  cent  of  its  initial  value.  Thereafter,  the  impulses  are  discharged 
in  groups,  which  gradually  merge  into  a  more  or  less  regular  train  when 
the  frequency  reaches  about  150  per  second.  Such  a  gradual  increase 
in  the  frequency  of  impulses  is  shown  in  one  of  the  curves  of  figure  6. 

The  development  of  activity  is  not  always  so  gradual.  Sometimes, 
the  initial  frequency  may  be  high,  and  then  decline  to  a  lower  level 
that  is  sustained  for  some  hours,  with  the  development  of  hundreds  of 


464 


ANNALS  NEW  YORK  ACADEMY  OF  SCIENCES 


200 


> 
u 

Z 

u 

D 
Of 

u 
q: 
ii. 


O  10  20 

MINUTES 

Figure  6.  Time-course  of  discharge  (impulses  per  second  in  a  single  fiber),  after  topical  appli- 
cation of  isotonic  solution  containing  sodium  citrate  (35  mM)  and  sodium  chloride. 

Open  circles :  gradual  increase  of  frequency  during  first  period  of  chemical  excitation. 

Squares:  response  to  second  application  of  same  solution,  after  intervening  2  hours  in  reference 
fluid.  ^   .  , 

Filled  circles:  response  to  a  third  application,  after  another  hour  in  Ringer  s  fluid. 


200 


0 


20 


40  60 

MINUTES 


80 


100 


FiatiBE  7.    The  discharge  of   impulses  continuing   at  high   frequency  for  many  minutes  during 
topical  application  of  isotonic  sodium  citrate.    Approximately  540,000  impulses  were  produced  by 
the  single  fiber,  during  the  activity  plotted  in  this  figure. 


BRINK  AND  OTHERS:  CHEMICAL  EXCITATION  OF  NERVE    465 

thousands  of  impulses  (figure  7).  This  initial  high  frequency  dis- 
charge usually  occurs  after  a  previous  period  of  chemical  excitation  that 
had  been  arrested  by  the  restoration  of  calcium  (figure  6).  It  also 
occurs  during  the  actual  restoration  of  calcium.^ 

A  nerve  which  has  been  once  modified  by  the  withdrawal  of  cal- 
cium continues  to  give  such  a  response  of  high  initial  frequency  to  a 
successive  activation,  even  though  it  has  been  in  unmodified  Ringer's 
fluid  for  many  hours  (figure  6).  Whether  the  impulses  start  at  a  high 
frequency  that  declines,  or  whether  the  frequency  gradually  increases, 
the  final,  sustained  average  frequency  is  about  the  same  for  a  given 
stimulating  fluid.  This  frequency  is,  to  an  important  degree,  deter- 
mined by  the  calcium  content  of  the  nerve,  and  our  experiments  also 
suggest  that  it  is,  in  part,  dependent  upon  the  rate  of  removal  of  cal- 
cium. This  latter  factor  may  be  especially  important  in  the  determina- 
tion of  the  transient  changes  of  frequency. 

Ill 

The  resting  metabolism  of  nerve  has  long  been  thought  of  as  neces- 
sary for  maintaining  the  organization  of  its  unstable  structure  against 
the  tendency  of  the  structure  to  become  disorganized.     In  accordance 
with  this  view,  the  less  stable  structure  resulting  from  the  withdrawal 
of  calcium  should  have  a  higher  metabolic  requirement  for  its  main- 
tenance.    This  increased  metabolism  has  been  observed.^"    We,  too, 
have  made  such  measurements  of  the  oxygen  consumption  of  nerve 
from  which  varying  amounts  of  calcium  have  been  withdrawn,  while, 
at  the  same  time,  measuring  the  excitability  and  recording  any  impulses 
that  were  initiated."     Alterations  of  calcium  content  in  the  nerve,  suffi- 
cient to  cause  a  lowered  threshold  to  electric  stimuli,  but  insufficient  to 
cause  the  rhythmic  discharge  of  impulses,  induce  an  increased  oxygen 
consumption   (figures  8  and  9).    As  the  calcium  content  is  further 
reduced,  the  oxygen  consumption  increases  still  more.     Finally,  a  level 
of  calcium  content  may  be  reached  which  is  sufficiently  low  to  cause 
the  rhythmic  discharge  of  impulses,  and  associated  with  this  calcium 
content  there  is  a  still  higher  oxygen  consumption  (figure  8) . 

This  progressive  increase- of  oxygen  consumption  of  nerve  with  de- 
creasing concentrations  of  calcium,  starting  at  calcium  levels  too  great 
to  permit  the  development  of  spontaneous  activity,  raises  the  question 
as  to  the  meaning  of  the  term,  "resting  oxygen  consumption."  The 
oxygen  consumption  of  axons  which  are  resting,  in  the  sense  of  not 

conducting   impulses,   may   be   quite   different  in   different   chemical 
environments. 


46G 


ANNALS  NEW  YORK  ACADEMY  OF  SCIENCES 


NO   ACTIVITY 


Ob 


10 


15 


20 


CA      WILLIMOLCS    PER      LITER 


Figure  8.  The  respiration  (as  per  cent  of  value  in  reference  solution)  of  a  frog  sciatic  nerve 
increasing  when  the  concentration  of  calcium  chloride  in  bathing  fluid  is  below  1.0  mM.  There 
is  an  appreciable  increase  at  a  concentration  which  is  not  low  enough  to  initiate  impulses. 


300 


50 
Threshold 


Figure  9.  Respiration  of  frog  sciatic  nerve  in  relation  to  threshold  of  a  fibers  in  same  nerve. 
Changes  produced  by  equilibration  in  isosmotic  solutions  containing  1.0,  0.5,  and  0.25  mM  CaCh. 
Both  respiration  and  threshold  (rheobase)  are  expressed  as  per  cent  of  their  values  (marked  bj 
solid  square)  in  reference  solution  (containing  2.0  mM  CaCh). 

Open  circles :  A  nei-ve  showing  a  small  per  cent  change  in  respiration  has  a  correspondingly  small 
per  cent  change  in  rheobase. 

Solid  cu'cles :  Another  nerve,  showing  a  large  per  cent  change  in  respiration,  has  a  large  per 
cent  change  in  rheobase. 


BRINK  AND  OTHERS:  CHEMICAL  EXCITATION  OF  NERVE    467 


An  increased  rate  of  oxidation  is  essential  for  the  initiation  of  im- 
pulses. That  is  shown  by  the  effects  of  an  oxidation-inhibiting  agent, 
such  as  sodium  azide.  A  portion  of  a  nerve  trunk  was  placed  in  sodium 
citrate,  and  impulses  which  were  thus  developed  were  recorded  in  one 
of  the  fibers  coming  from  the  chemically  activated  region.  Sodium 
azide  was  then  applied  to  the  citrate-treated  portion  of  the  nerve,  in 
a  concentration  that  was  sufficient  to  suppress  the  chemical  excitation. 
For  this,  a  concentration  of  azide  which  restored  the  oxidation  to  a 
normal  rate  was  adequate  (figure  10).     After  the  rhythmic  discharge 


300 


200 


a. 


100  - 


AZIDE 


2  3 

MILLIMOLES    PER     LITER 


Figure   10.    The  respiration  of  a   calcium-deficient  nei-ve   (per  cent  of  value  in  reference  solu- 
tion) suppressed  by  sodium  azide,  as  is  the  respiration  of  a  normal  nerve. 

of  impulses  had  thus  been  inhibited,  it  was  still  possible,  for  several 
hours  to  send  a  high  frequency  train  of  impulses,  initiated  by  electric 
stimuli,  through  the  calcium-deficient  and  azide-treated  length  of  nerve. 
The  initiation  of  impulses  by  a  calcium-deficient  region  of  nerve  re- 
quires a  higher  rate  of  oxidation  than  is  necessary  for  the  maintenance 
of  the  capacity  of  such  a  calcium-deficient  region  to  conduct  impulses. 
Certain  specific  chemical  changes  in  the  constitution  of  nerve  and  in- 
creased metabolic  rate  both  appear  to  be  necessary  for  the  initiation  of 
rhythmic  activity.  One  without  the  other  is  an  inadequate  condition 
for  self-excitation. 


468  ANNALS  NEW  YORK  ACADEMY  OF  SCIENCES 

Although  there  are  many  instances  of  a  close  parallelism  between 
the  stability  of  nerve,  as  measured  by  its  electric  threshold  or  by  the 
spontaneous  development  of  impulses,  and  its  rate  of  respiration,  there 
are  exceptions.  For  instance,  the  rhythmic  activity  may  be  abolished 
by  the  application  of  potassium  chloride,  which,  at  the  same  time,  in- 
creases the  rate  of  respiration.  Another  instance  of  such  a  lack  of 
parallelism  is  revealed  when  a  nerve  is  returned  to  its  normal  fluid 
environment,  after  treatment  with  sodium  citrate.  The  rhythmic  ac- 
tivity is  promptly  suppressed,  and  the  threshold  becomes  normal,  long 
before  there  is  a  corresponding  recovery  of  the  original,  normal  rate 
of  respiration. 

IV 

There  has  been  a  persistent  notion  that  the  initiation  of  trains  of 
impulses  from  a  chemically  activated  portion  of  an  axon  or  from  a 
sense  organ  under  a  constant  stimulus  is  due  to  a  gradient  of  electric 
potential  at  the  site  where  the  impulses  originate.  Indeed,  Adrian^^ 
found  that  there  was  a  gradient  of  10  mV  between  a  normal  portion  of 
nerve  and  an  injured  region  from  which  impulses  were  discharged. 
Accordingly,  he  attributed  the  excitation  to  this  demarcation  potential. 
Furthermore,  Erlanger  and  Blair^*  and  Fessard^^  caused  the  rhythmic 
discharge  of  impulses  by  the  passage  of  constant  currents.  Finally, 
Katz^*^  and  Arvanitaki^^  found  that  the  duration  of  such  an  electrically 
induced  repetitive  discharge  could  be  much  prolonged  by  reducing  the 
calcium  content  of  the  nerve. 

Because  of  these  considerations,  we  have  carefully  searched  for  some 
causal  relation  between  a  potential  gradient  developed  at  the  site  of 
calcium  removal  and  the  chemical  initiation  of  impulses.  To  do  this, 
one  of  a  pair  of  non-polarizable  electrodes  was  placed  in  contact  with 
the  chemically  altered  region  of  the  nerve;  the  second  was  in  contact 
with  an  adjacent,  untreated  portion.  We  have  found  that  the  activity 
develops  in  the  calcium-deficient  part  of  the  nerve,  without  the  appear- 
ance of  an  appreciable  longitudinal  potential  gradient.^^  However,  this 
part  of  the  nerve  is  very  sensitive  to  weak  currents.  It  is,  therefore, 
possible  that  potential  differences  of  less  than  a  millivolt  might  be  in- 
volved in  the  mechanism  of  chemical  excitation. 

Accordingly,  experiments  were  devised  to  study  quantitatively  the 
relation  between  the  frequency  of  conducted  impulses  and  changes  in 
membrane  polarization  produced  in  the  hyper-excitable  portion  of  the 
axon  by  certain  additional  chemical  agents.  Increasing  the  proportion 
of  potassium  chloride  in  the  solution  of  sodium  citrate  used  to  excite 


BRINK  AND  OTHERS:  CHEMICAL  EXCITATION  OF  NERVE    469 


the  nerve  makes  the  calcium-deficient  region  negative  to  the  adjacent 
parts  of  the  cell.  Under  these  conditions,  the  associated  current  flow 
is  inward  across  the  plasma  membrane  in  the  hyper-excitable  region 
of  the  axon.  The  conducted  impulses  still  occur,  but  at  a  reduced 
frequency,  compared  with  the  activity  initiated  by  a  solution  contain- 
ing less  potassium  chloride.     The  frequency  of  response  is  lower,  the 


3 

=!       -I 


o 

kl 
«> 

200 

a. 

CO 
UJ 

100 

s 


20  30 

MINUTES 

FiGUBE  11.  The  depression  by  KCl  of  the  activity  produced  by  topical  application  of  isotonic 
sodium  citrate  is  related  to  the  associated  decrease  in  membrane  polarization. 

Upper  curve  is  potential  difference  between  treated  region  of  nerve  and  normal  part  of  nerve. 
At  the  first  arrow,  the  solution  was  changed  from  isotonic  sodium  citrate  to  one  containing  the 
same  amount  of  citrate,  but  with  15  mM  K  in  place  of  some  of  the  sodium.  At  the  second 
arrow,  the  ner\'e  was  returned  to  a  solution  containing  isotonic  sodium  citrate  without  potassium. 
At  the  third  arrow,  the  solution  containing  potassium  was  again  applied  to  the  nerve. 

The  lower  curve  shows  change  in  frequency  of  response  recorded  from  a  single  fiber  in  this 
nerve.  A  similar  suppression  of  activity  during  the  first  cycle  of  depolarization  was  observed,  but 
not  recorded. 

higher  the  concentration  of  potassium  chloride,  and,  therefore,  de- 
creases, as  the  degree  of  depolarization  increases.  When  the  calcium- 
deficient  part  of  the  cell  is  made  sufficiently  negative,  the  activity  is 
suppressed,  but  it  begins  again  as  this  depolarization  is  removed  by 
washing  out  the  potassium  chloride  (figure  11).  Conversely,  if  this 
region  of  the  nerve  is  made  positive  to  adjacent  parts,  as  by  a  solution 
of  sodium  thiocyanate,  the  frequency  of  the  impulses  is  increased. 

Thus,  small  differences  of  potential  between  a  normal  and  hyper- 
irritable  region  of  an  axon  modify  the  frequency  of  the  impulses  dis- 


470 


ANNALS  NEW  YORK  ACADEMY  OF  SCIENCES 


charged  from  that  region,  despite  the  fact  that  the  impulses  originate 
without  the  mediation  of  such  potential  gradients.  The  influence  of 
these  gradients  on  the  excitability  of  calcium-deficient  nerve,  as  meas- 
ured by  changes  in  the  frequency  of  impulse  discharge,  is  in  agreement 
with  the  usual  effects  of  current  flow  from  an  external  source:  depres- 
sion at  the  anode  and  excitation  at  the  cathode. 

Although  the  chemical  excitation  of  nerve  does  not  depend  upon  the 
development  of  a  steady  current  flow,  the  discharge  of  chemically 
initiated  impulses  can  be  modified  by  an  externally  imposed  potential 


§250 

o 
u 

UJ 
(O 

cm 
h!200 

UJ 

-J 

a. 
ZI50 


100 


i 


POLARIZE 


RECORD 


10 


20 


30 


40 


50 


SECONDS 


Figure  12.  The  frequency  of  response  in  a  single  fiber  stimulated  by  topical  application  of 
isotonic  sodium  citrate  is  reduced  wlien  the  treated  region  is  anodally  polarized  (first  arrow). 
When  circuit  is  opened  (second  arrow),  there  is  a  transient  increase  in  response.  The  current 
passed  into  nerve  in  the  treated  region  as  shown  in  the  diagram. 

gradient.  This  was  first  reported  by  Fessard,^^  who  observed  such  an 
effect  in  crab  nerves  which  had  been  excited  to  activity  by  the  applica- 
tion of  alcohol  or  sodium  thiocyanate.  To  study  this  problem  further, 
we  have  passed  a  polarizing  current  through  a  calcium-depleted  region 
of  frog  nerve,  during  the  period  of  constant  frequency  of  discharge. 
When  the  direction  of  current  flow  is  such  that  it  enters  the  nerve 
fiber  in  the  chemically  activated  region,  the  average  frequency  of  im- 
pulses is  reduced  for  a  brief  time.  As  shown  in  figure  12,  only  a  slight 
depression  continues  after  a  few  seconds.  When  the  polarizing  current 
is  terminated,  there  is  a  temporary  increase  in  the  frequency  of  im- 
pulses from  the  chemically  activated  region,  followed  by  a  return  to 
the  frequency  that  preceded  the  beginning  of  the  current  flow. 


BRINK  AND  OTHERS:  CHEMICAL  EXCITATION  OF  NERVE    471 

If  the  current  is  in  the  opposite  direction,  so  that  it  flows  out  of  the 
fiber  in  the  calcium-deficient  region,  the  sequence  of  frequency  changes 
is  reversed.  Then,  as  the  current  starts,  there  is  a  transient  increase 
of  frequency,  followed  by  a  temporary  depression  when  the  current  is 
interrupted. 

Still  obscure  are  the  cellular  mechanisms  which  account  for  an  in- 
crease in  the  frequency  of  impulses  from  calcium-deficient  nerve,  when 
positively  charged  ions  move  outward  across  the  fiber  interface,  or  for 


-130  L 


Figure  13.  The  maximum  change  in  frequency  (impulses  per  second)  in  a  chemically  excited 
frog  a  fiber  is  proportional  to  the  magnitude  of  polarizing  current  (in  microamperes).  Nerve  ex- 
cited as  described  in  figure  12. 

Positive  current :   anode  in  the  treated  region ;    negative  current :   cathode  in  treated  region. 

a  decrease  of  frequency,  when  the  ionic  movement  is  reversed.  The 
effects  are,  however,  consistent  with  the  long-established  fact  that  a 
cathodally  polarized  region  of  nerve  is  more  irritable,  while  anodally 
polarized  nerve  is  less  irritable.  Accordingly,  the  effects  are  also  in 
agreement  with  the  view  that  an  agent  which  reduces  the  stability  of 
the  nerve  structure  increases  the  frequency  of  chemically  induced 
activity. 

The  magnitude  of  the  transient  increase  or  decrease  in  the  frequency 
of  impulses,  caused  by  the  passage  of  an  electric  current  through  a 
chemically  activated  nerve,  depends  upon  the  strength  of  current.  Fig- 
ure 13  shows  that  there  is,  indeed,  a  linear  relationship  between  the 
current  strength  and  the  maximal  increase  or  decrease  of  impulse  fre- 
quency caused  by  the  current  flow,  within  certain  limits.     This  figure 


472 


ANNALS  NEW  YORK  ACADEMY  OF  SCIENCES 


also  reveals  the  significant  fact  that  there  is,  apparently,  no  minimal 
current  strength  that  must  be  exceeded  before  the  rhythmic  discharge 
from  the  chemically  sensitized  nerve  is  modified.  Any  change  in  the 
direct  current,  no  matter  how  small,  flowing  across  the  membrane  of 
these  chemically  modified  nerves,  alters  the  rhythmic  activity  of  the 
nerve  and  is  reflected  in  the  altered  frequency  of  the  propagated  im- 
pulses. This  is  in  contrast  to  the  limiting  threshold  of  current 
strength  necessary  for  the  excitation  of  a  conducted  impulse  in  a  nerve 
with  normal  calcium  content. 


We  have  already  said  that  potassium  chloride  causes  a  decrease  in  the 
frequency  of  the  impulses  developed  in  a  calcium-deficient  portion  of 


o  100  - 

z 

8 

Ul 
(0 

(T 
bJ 
Q. 

m 

bJ 
O) 

-I 

2 

40  60 

CONCENTRATION 

Figure  14.  Frequency  of  response  due  to  action  of  isotonic  solutions  containing  various  propor- 
tions of  sodium  chloride  and  sodium  citrate.  Concentration  in  terms  of  per  cent  of  isotonic  solu- 
tion of  sodium  citrate. 

a  nerve.  This  is  the  effect  of  potassium  chloride  when  its  action  has 
reached  a  steady  state,  but  it  is  preceded  by  a  transient  increase  in  the 
number  of  impulses  discharged  per  second.  This  stimulating  action  of 
potassium  chloride  also  occurs  in  nerves  with  normal  calcium  content, 
but  there  it  is  of  even  shorter  duration.  Both  the  maximum  frequency 
and  the  duration  of  the  impulse  discharge,  caused  by  an  increase  of 
potassium  chloride,  are  greater,  the  lower  the  calcium  content  of  the 
fluid  bathing  the  nerve. -°  Thus,  the  removal  of  calcium  from  a  nerve 
makes  it  more  sensitive  to  the  transient  stimulating  action  of  a  moder- 
ate increase  in  the  concentration  of  potassium  chloride. 

This  increased  sensitivity  of  calcium-deficient  nerve  to  other  chem- 
ical agents  is  further  revealed  in  the  experiments  shown  in  figure  14. 
There,  the  nerve  was  made  active  by  bathing  a  portion  of  it  in  isotonic 


BRINK  AND  OTHERS:  CHEMICAL  EXCITATION  OF  NERVE    473 

sodium  chloride.  When  the  frequency  of  response  had  become  con- 
stant, the  sodium  chloride  solution  was  replaced  by  one  in  which  some 
of  the  sodium  chloride  had  been  replaced  by  sodium  citrate.  The  fre- 
quency of  impulse  discharge  then  increased,  as  is  shown.  AVith  each 
further  increase  in  the  proportion  of  sodium  citrate,  there  was  a  further 
increase  in  the  average  number  of  impulses  per  second.  When  the 
nerve  was  subsequently  returned  to  solutions  containing  successively 
smaller  concentrations  of  citrate,  there  was  a  parallel  decrease  in  the 
impulse  frequency.  Finally,  in  isotonic  sodium  chloride,  the  initial 
low  degree  of  activity  was  resumed.  This  decrease  in  average  fre- 
quency, associated  with  the  return  to  isotonic  sodium  chloride,  is  obvi- 
ously not  due  to  a  restoration  of  calcium  to  the  nerve.  It  seems  prob- 
able, therefore,  that  a  nerve  made  active  by  removal  of  calcium  is  sen- 
sitive to  changes  in  the  concentration  of  the  citrate  ion.  This  contrasts 
with  the  previously  mentioned  lack  of  effect  of  citrate  upon  a  nerve  in 
the  presence  of  Ringer's  proportion  of  calcium  ions. 

In  a  similar  manner,  the  stimulating  action  of  sodium  thiocyanate 
is  enhanced  by  first  removing  some  of  the  calcium  from  the  nerve. 
Also,  tetraethyl  ammonium  chloride  will  stimulate  frog  nerve,^^  and 
we  have  found  that  its  effectiveness  in  initiating  impulses  is  greater, 
if  the  axon  is  sensitized  by  preliminary  removal  of  some  of  the  calcium. 

Another  quaternary  ammonium  salt  of  interest  in  this  discussion 
is  acetylcholine.  Lorente  de  No  has  shown^^  that  it  does  not  alter  the 
membrane  potential  of  frog  nerve,  even  in  massive  concentrations.  On 
the  other  hand,  Nachmansohn  argues  for  the  possibility  of  such  an  ac- 
tion, on  the  grounds  that  cholinesterase-inhibiting  agents,  which  should 
permit  the  accumulation  of  acetylcholine,  do  cause  depolarization  of 
squid  nerve. ^^  In  our  experience,  acetylcholine  does  not  induce  a  dis- 
charge of  impulses  when  it  is  applied  to  the  axons  of  a  peripheral  frog 
nerve  trunk,  and  we  have  not  been  able  to  increase  the  frequency  of 
chemically  excited  impulses  by  adding  acetylcholine  to  the  calcium- 
deficient  fluid.  Also,  we  have  investigated  the  effects  of  this  substance 
on  mammalian  nerve,  by  perfusing  the  stellate  ganglion  of  a  cat.  In  no 
case  have  we  found  any  evidence  that  impulses  are  thus  initiated  in  the 
pre-synaptic  fibers  within  the  ganglion,  even  though  as  much  as  500 
micro-grams  of  acetylcholine  were  added  to  each  cc.  of  perfusion  fluid. 
This  was  determined  by  observing  that  no  impulses  were  discharged 
over  the  fibers  of  the  preganghonic  trunk  (figure  15).  Finally,  there 
remains  the  contrasting  and  significant  observation,  that  much  lower 
concentrations  of  acetylcholine  do  cause  the  discharge  of  rhythmically 
recurring  impulses  in  the  post-synaptic  neurons.     The  cell  bodies  or  the 


474 


ANNALS  NEW  YORK  ACADEMY  OF  SCIENCES 


POSTGANGLIONIC  ^  iJi^kUiitLi^tllM  k  *^^^^ 

NERVE         mmm^>ikmmm»m*  i^^nfff^Wm^ .p     '0° 


ACETYLCHOLINE 


^G. /CC. 


PREGANGLIONIC 
NERVE 


MM^MIMMMMWMMMMMHMt)  MMMMMMIMIWII 


^ACETYLCHOLINE 

100  \iQ>JCC. 


PREGANGLIONIC 


ACETYLCHOLINE 


NtnVt  "^  oOOpG./cc. 


PREGANGLIONIC 
NERVE 


*/mf0if^i0mmimmm 


SODIUM  CITRATE 
380  uG. /cc. 


Figure  15.  Responses  recorded  from  the  preganglionic  and  postganglionic  nerves  of  a  cat's 
stellate  sympathetic  ganglion,  during  perfusion  with  acetylcholine  and  sodium  citrate.  Control 
records   in  the  absence  of  a  chemical   excitant  in  the  left  hand  column.     Time:  0.1   sec. 

immediately  contiguous  portions  of  their  axons  differ  from  axons  in 
general  in  some  way  that  makes  them  sensitive  to  the  action  of  this 
agent. 

This  is  a  striking  example  of  a  specificity  of  nerve  structure  involved 
in  the  process  of  chemical  excitation.  It  has  been  suggested  that  the 
basis  of  this  differential  action  is  the  presence  or  absence  of  a  myelin 
sheath  that  would  prevent  the  rapid  penetration  of  the  acetylcholine." 
That  is  not  likely  to  be  the  explanation  of  the  contrasting  effects  in  the 
experiments  just  reported,  for  the  terminal  portions  of  the  pre-synaptic 
fibers  within  the  ganglion  are  considered  to  be  non-myelinated/"*  as 
are  the  post-synaptic  neurons. 

Certain  chemical  agents,  such  as  acetylcholine,  are  highly  specific, 
with  regard  to  the  type  of  nerve  structure  they  excite.  Others,  of  which 
citrate  and  calcium-deficient  solutions  are  examples,  are  quite  general 
in  their  action.^^  Thus,  a  reduction  of  calcium  ions  in  the  perfusing 
fluid,  or  the  addition  of  sodium  citrate,  causes  the  discharge  of  recurring 
impulses  in  both  the  pre-synaptic  and  the  post-synaptic  neurons,  as  in 
axons  generally  (figure  15). 

The  character  of  the  response  of  ganglion  cells  to  acetylcholine  is,  in 
many  respects,  analogous  to  the  response  of  peripheral  axons  to  chem- 
ical excitation.  For  each  cell,  there  is  a  threshold  concentration  that 
must  be  exceeded  before  impulses  are  developed.  This  threshold  dif- 
fers from  cell  to  cell,  but  it  is  usually  less  than  25  micrograms  of  acetyl- 


BRINK  AND  OTHERS:  CHEMICAL  EXCITATION  OF  NERVE    475 

choline  per  cc,  during  continuous  perfusion  with  solutions  containing  no 
inhibitor  of  cholinesterase.  When  the  threshold  for  a  cell  is  exceeded, 
it  discharges  impulses  with  a  regular  rhythm,  and  for  extended  periods 
that  we  have  observed  to  be  as  long  as  an  hour.     The  frequency  of  this 


B 


Figure  16.  Impulses  discharged  from  a  single  sympathetic  ganglion  cell  in  response  to  acetyl- 
choline in  concentrations  of  A:25;  B  :50 ;  C:100  micrograms  per  cc.  Cat's  stellate  ganglion  per- 
fused with  a  modified  Ringer's  fluid  containing  acet.\lcholine,  but  no  inhibitor  of  cholinesterase. 
Impulses  recorded  from  a  fine  strand  of  the  postganglionic  nerve.    Time  in  seconds. 

discharge  increases  with  increased  concentrations  of  acetylcholine  (fig- 
ure 16) .  Finally,  at  concentrations  of  about  200  micrograms  per  cc, 
the  excitatory  action  ceases,  the  discharge  of  impulses  is  arrested,  and 
the  ganglion  cells  cannot  be  stimulated  by  volleys  of  preganglionic 
impulses. 

The  response  of  nerve  to  chemical  excitants  depends  upon  the  totality 
of  environmental  agents.  This  has  been  emphasized  before.  It  is  a 
fact  that  is  illustrated  by  the  effects  of  the  combined  action  of  acetyl- 


476 


ANNALS  NEW  YORK  ACADEMY  OF  SCIENCES 


choline  and  other  chemical  agents  on  ganglion  cells.  For  example,  the 
frequency  of  impulses  initiated  by  a  certain  concentration  of  acetyl- 
choline may  be  reduced  by  increasing  the  concentration  of  calcium  in 
the  perfusion  fluid,  or  by  reducing  the  concentration  of  potassium  (fig- 
ure 17).     Conversely,  the  frequency  of  discharge  may  be  increased  by 


2  X   NORMAL 
CALCIUM 


5  X    NORMAL 
POTASSIUM 


i«iw 


'■°'""'-iiiii|i|iiiiiii  liiii  II  mill  mil  mil 


NORMAL 


!ilillllillllllllllllilli||l|lllll|llllllllli44^ 


4 


NO    CALCIUM 


NO     POTASSIUM 


Figure  17.  Impulses  discharged  from  a  single  cell  in  a  cat's  stellate  ganglion,  during  perfusion 
with  six  different  solutions,  all  containing  the  same  amounts  of  acetylcholine  (40  /xg.  1  cc),  but 
different  concentrations  of  calcium  and   potassium.     Time   in  seconds. 

lowering  the  concentration  of  calcium  or  by  augmenting  the  concentra- 
tion of  potassium.  These  effects  of  calcium  and  potassium  on  the 
rhythmic  action  initiated  by  another  chemical  agent  might  be  antici- 
pated from  our  knowledge  of  their  effects  on  the  electrical  excitability 
of  axons.  It  is,  perhaps,  worthy  of  comment  that,  regardless  of  whether 
the  acetylcholine,  calcium,  and  potassium  act  upon  the  same  or  different 
parts  of  the  irritable  mechanism,  their  combined  effects  become  mani- 
fest in  a  modification  of  the  rhythmic  process  which  initiates  the 
propagated  impulses. 

This  repetitive  process,  which  is  a  latent  characteristic  of  nerve,  is 
revealed  in  the  discharge  of  impulses  initiated  by  sensory  stimulation 
or  by  chemical  action.  It  is  also  to  be  observed  in  the  periodic  activity 
of  nerve  cells  which  are  excited  by  the  arrival  of  impulses  in  adjacent 
pre-synaptic  endings.  When  the  preganglionic  fibers  entering  a  sym- 
pathetic ganglion  are  stimulated  by  repetitive  electrical  shocks  of  high 
frequency,  the  cells  discharge  repetitively,  but  at  a  much  lower  fre- 


BRINK  AND  OTHERS:  CHEMICAL  EXCITATION  OF  NERVE    477 

quency.  Furthermore,  the  cells  di.scharge  their  impulses  in  no  fixed 
temporal  relation  to  the  incoming,  excitatory  impulses.  In  short,  the 
cells  which  are  activated  through  tiie  pre-synaptic  endings  initiate  im- 
pulses at  a  Irequency  that  depends  upcjn  the  characteristics  of  each  cell, 
as  well  as  upon  the  frequency  and   tlie  number  of  pre-synaptic  im- 


FiGURE  18.  Impulses  discharged  by  a  few  sympathetic  ganglion  cells  in  response  to  stimu- 
lation of  the  preganglionic  nerve  at  a  frequency  of  50  per  second.  The  ganglion  was  perfused 
with   a  modified   Ringer's  solution  containing  various  amounts  of  calcium  chloride: 

Uppermost  record,  4.4  mM  ;  middle  record,  2.2  mM  ;  bottom  record,  1.1  mM.  The  middle  rec- 
ord represents  the  normal  level  of  calcium. 

,Time  in  0.1  seconds. 

pulses.^'  If  these  cellular  characteristics  are  modified  by  any  means 
(by  nerve  impulses  or  by  chemical  agents),  the  rhythmic  processes  are 
altered,  and  this  modifies  the  frequency  of  their  action. 

Such  a  modification  of  the  rhythmic  response  of  a  nerve  cell  to  neural 
activation  can  be  accomplished,  as  would  be  expected,  by  varying  the 
concentrations  of  calcium  ions  in  the  synaptic  regions  (figure  18).  If 
activity  is  excited  in  a  ganglion  cell  by  trains  of  pre-synaptic  impulses, 
the  frequency  is  decreased  by  raising  the  concentration  ojf  calcium  in 
the  perfusion  fluid.     Alternatively,  the  frequency  of  the  impulses  dis-- 


478  ANNALS  NEW  YORK  ACADEMY  OF  SCIENCES 

charged  from  the  gangUon  cells  can  be  increased  by  decreasing  the  cal- 
cium concentration.  It  must  be  said  that  a  further  reduction  of  cal- 
cium in  the  perfusing  solution  may  cause  a  complete  block  of  ganglion 
cell  excitation  by  impuls3s  in  the  pre-synaptic  fibers. 

VI 

Soms  evidence  regarding  the  nature  of  the  cellular  events  which 
cause  the  more  or  less  rhythmic  discharge  of  impulses  from  a  chemically 
excited  region  of  nerve  can  be  derived  from  a  consideration  of  the 
temporal  distribution  of  the  impulses.  Such  a  study  suggests  that  there 
is  a  rhythmic  excitatory  process  in  nerve,  of  a  fairly  constant  frequency, 
which  may  or  may  not  produce  an  impulse  each  cycle.  Definite  evi- 
dence from  several  sources  is  now  available  for  the  existence  of  such  a 
process.  The  role  it  plays  in  the  regulation  of  the  frequency  of  con- 
ducted impulses  will  be  discussed  in  the  following  pages. 

The  earliest  work  on  the  discharge  of  impulses  in  single  neurons  re- 
vealed a  temporal  distribution  of  impulses  that  was  more  or  less  regu- 
lar, but  not  quite  periodic.  Thus,  one  of  us  in  1928,  when  commenting 
on  the  failure  of  the  discharge  from  a  fatigued  muscle  tension  receptor, 
remarked  that  "one  or  more  impulses  drop  out  of  an  otherwise  fairly 
regular  series,  the  impulses  becoming  more  and  more  scattered. "^^  The 
longer  intervals  were  observed  to  be  approximately  equal  multiples  of 
the  shortest  time  interval  between  successive  impulses.  Adrian  observed 
a  similar  phenomenon  in  the  discharge  of  injured  mammalian  nerve 
fibers, ^^  and  such  irregular  intermittence  appears  in  Pumphrey's"^  rec- 
ords of  impulses  from  taste  receptors.  More  recently,  this  occasional 
omission  of  impulses  from  an  otherwise  regular  series  was  observed  dur- 
ing the  repetitive  discharge  caused  by  super-threshold  direct  current 
excitation  (Erlanger  and  Blair,"  Fessard^^). 

This  same  irregularity  in  the  temporal  distribution  of  impulses  is  a 
prominent  characteristic  in  our  experiments  upon  chemically  excited 
axons.  This  will  have  been  evident  in  some  of  the  preceding  records, 
but,  for  the  more  precise  analysis  of  this  phenomenon,  additional  ex- 
periments will  be  presented.  The  fibers  were  excited  by  removing  Ca""* 
from  a  short  length  of  nerve,  by  means  of  sodium  citrate,  as  previously 
described.  The  measurements  were  made  on  records  taken  when  the 
nerve  was  producing  impulses  at  a  constant  average  frequency.  Under 
these  circumstances,  the  temporal  distribution  may  be  regular  or  ir- 
regular. 

The  magnitudes  of  the  time  intervals  between  successive  impulses  in 
^  certain  series  obtained  in  the  above  manner  are  plotted  in  figure  19, 


BRINK  AND  OTHERS:  CHEMICAL  EXCITATION  OF  NERVE    479 


as  a  function  of  the  number  of  the  impulse  in  the  sequence.  Obviously, 
the  intervals  are  grouped  about  certain  values,  which  are  6,  12,  and  18 
milliseconds.  These  same  data  are  presented  in  the  form  of  a  dis- 
tribution plot  in  FIGURE  20,  where  the  number  of  intervals  in  a  certain 
range  is  plotted  against  the  length  of  the  interval.  Since  the  ordinate 
is  a  measure  of  the  probability  of  occurrence  of  the  interval  indicated 

52- 

"I- 
30 
29 
;6 
27 
26 
29 
24 
23 
22 
21 
n  20 

a 

-  19 

S  '■' 

f  12 


•  •   •••  •. 


•  •  •     • 


■■■■''■''■' ■  '  ' '  ■ 


30  40  50 

NUMBER   OF    IMPULSE    IN    TRAIN 


I  I  I  I  I  I  I  1  I  I  I  I  I  I  I 


Figure  19.  The  intPirals  bptv.een  impulses  ipiorded  from  a  chemirallv  exritp<I  .=infr'e  filipr 
(fiog)  .Tre  aplHoxiiiKitelv  intpfrial  in  I't.p'ps  <if  ;i  !r;ist  intni\nl.  In  lliis  filipr,  the  least  interval 
was  about  6  milliseconds.     Stimulation  by  localized  rcmoxal  of  calcium  fiom  the  axon. 

on  the  axis  of  abscissae,  it  is  obvious  that  all  intervals  are  not  equally 
probable.  The  most  probable  values  are  6,  12,  and  18  milliseconds. 
In  all  the  frog  fibers  thus  far  examined,  the  most  probable  values  of 
the  least  interval  are  in  the  range  of  3-6  milliseconds. 

The  aliquot  relations  between  these  most  probable  values  for  Ihc 
fiber  just  cited  suggest,  again,  that  the  longer  intervals  are  due  to  the 
omission  of  one  or  more  impulses  from  an  otherwise  continuous  series. 
Such  omissions  could  be  due  to  failure  of  the  impulses  to  be  initiated  in 
the  chemically  excited  region,  or  to  blocked  conduction  between  that 
region  and  the  recording  electrodes.  If  the  nerve  is  stimulated  at 
high  frequency  by  repeated  electric  shocks,  the  impulses  travel  over 
the  nerve  fiber  and  through  the  treated  region.  Consequently,  there 
is  no  reason  why  each  impulse  initiated  by  chemical  excitation  should 
not,  likewise,  be  conducted  to  the  recording  electrodes.     We  conclude, 


480 


ANNALS  NEW  YORK  ACADEMY  OF  SCIENCES 


therefore,  tliat  tlie  omission  of  impulses  from  the  series  is  not  due  to 
conduction  block.  The  longer  intervals  in  the  records  must  be  due  to 
failure  of  one  or  more  impulses  to  be  initiated. 


100  - 
90  - 

80  - 

70  - 

<n 

-J 

>60 

K 
UJ 

K 

?50 

ii. 
o 


UJ  ' 


30 


20  - 


10  - 


0  - 


J — I I I I l_i. 


>     '      (      I l_l I I — I — I — I — I — I — I    J.    < 


8         10        12         14         16 
DURATION  Of  INTERVAL  (MS.) 


18 


20       22        24 


FiGURG  20.  Diagram  representing  the  same  data  as  in  figure  19,  but  extended  to  over  500  suc- 
cessive intervals  between  impulses.  Ordinates  are  number  of  intervals  having  a  value  in  each  0.4 
millisecond  range.     Inten-als  longer  than  24  ms.  not  shown. 

It  is  difficult  to  account  for  these  observations,  except  on  the  assump- 
tion that  there  is,  in  this  nerve  fiber,  some  rhythmic  process,  with  an 
average  period  of  6  milliseconds,  that  maintains  its  rhythmic  quality  in- 
dependently of  the  initiation  of  impulses,  once  the  impulses  are  started. 

Arvanitaki^'  and  Hodgkin'^  have  presented  evidence  that  an  impulse, 
initiated  by  electrical  stimulation  in  unmyelinated  nerve,  develops 
from  a  local  electrical  response  which  occurs  at  the  site  of  stimulation. 
Arvanitaki  also  showed^^  that  this  local  response,  which  is  elicited  by 
electrical  stimulation,  may  be  cyclic  in  nerAcs  deprived  of  calcium. 
Using  the  giant  nerve  fiber  of  the  squid,  wo  have  studied  the  develop- 
ment of  this  local  electrical  response  in  nerves  excited  solely  by  the 
removal  of  calchnn.  We  had  two  objectives:  (1)  to  see  if  the  local 
response  appears  before  the  initiation  of  the  first  impulse  in  a  train, 
and  (2)  to  ascertain  whether  a  rhythmic  local  response  could  be  pro- 
duced chemically,  and  independently  of  conducted  impulses. 


BRINK  AND  OTHERS:  CHEMICAL  EXCITATION  OF  NERVE    481 

When  part  of  a  squid  giant  fiber  is  deprived  of  calcium  ions,  trains 
of  impulses  are  initiated  in  this  region  and  conducted  to  the  recording 
electrodes.  The  nerve  behaves,  in'  this  respect,  exactly  like  the 
myelinated  nerve  of  a  frog,  but  here  conditions  are  more  favorable  for 
recording  the  difference  of  potential  between  the  chemically  excited 
region  of  the  fiber  and  a  remote  portion.  When  this  is  done,  it  is  found 
that  the  conducted  impulses  are  preceded  by  a  series  of  local  periodic 
potential  changes  of  variable  amplitude  and  relatively  constant  fre- 
quency^^   (figure  21).     The  spacing  between  adjacent  peaks  of  the 


Figure  21.  Local  electrical  response,  recorded  from  a  chemically  excited  region  of  giant  axon 
of  squid,  is  oscillatory  and  precedes  the  conducted  impulses.  The  last  ten  oscillations  on  the  right 
of  the  record  initiated  propagated  impulses,  which  are  much  larger  in  amplitude  than  shown. 
Stimulation  by  topical  application  of  isotonic  sodium  chloride. 

local  response,  just  before  the  conducted  impulses  appear,  is  the  same 
as  the  spacing  between  the  conducted  impulses.  It  is  obvious,  as 
Arvanitaki  concluded,^^  that  the  frequency  of  conducted  impulses  along 
the  giant  axon  is  determined  by  the  frequency  of  the  local  excitatory 
process. 

If  relatively  little  calcium  is  removed  from  the  nerve,  local  periodic 
electric  changes  may  be  observed  which  do  not  initiate  propagated  im- 
pulses. The  local  process  is  an  essential  part  of  the  excitatory  mecha- 
nism, but  the  cyclic  changes  initiate  impulses  only  when  a  given  cycle 
is  of  sufficient  magnitude.  Furthermore,  the  frequency  of  the  local 
process  is  essentially  independent  of  whether  or  not  a  conducted  impulse 
is  initiated  by  each  cycle. 

Figure  22  gives  the  frequencies  of  conducted  impulses  observed  in 
nerves  treated  with  solutions  containing  different  concentrations  of  Ca"^, 
or  with  sodium  citrate.  The  frequencies  vary  from  250  to  400  cycles 
per  sec.  This  range  is  comparable  to  that  which  is  characteristic  of  the 
undamped  natural  frequency  of  the  nerve  membrane,  as  calculated  by 
Cole  and  Baker^^  from  impedance  measurements  on  squid  nerve.     This 


482 


ANNALS  NEW  YORK  ACADEMY  OF  SCIENCES 


parallelism  between  the  range  of  frequencies  in  the  local  rhythmic  re- 
sponse, the  fundamental  frequency  in  the  trains  of  chemically  initiated 
impulses,  and  the  undamped  natural  frequency  of  the  resting  nerve 
membrane,  support  Cole's  suggestion^*  that  it  is  the  structural  charac- 
teristics of  the  membrane  which  govern  the  periodic  activity  of  nerve. 

Two  frequencies  of  nerve  action  have  been  described  in  the  foregoing 
discussions  of  the  response  of  nerve  to  chemical  excitation.  One  is  the 
average  number  of  impulses  conducted  along  the  nerve  per  second;  the 
other  is  the  fundamental  and  relatively  constant  frequency  of  the 
excitatory  process,  which  has,  in  the  case  of  squid  nerve,  been  identi- 
fied with  the  local  electric  response. 


Nerve 

A 

B 

C 

5  nun* 
Calcium 

No 
Calcium 

No  Calcium. 
80  mm. 
Sodium  Citrate 

1 

260 

2 

250 

3 

300 

4 

250 

300 

2G0 

290 

400 

b 

260 

310 

6 

340 

7 

260 

290 

8 

300 

330 

9 

240 

10 

290 

Solutions  are  modified  sea  water.  Cal- 
cium and  magnesium  omitted  in  B  and  C. 
Magnesium  omitted  in  A.  KCl  concen- 
tration and  pH  are  same  as  in  sea  water. 

Figure  22.  Values  for  the  fundamental  frequency  obsei-ved  in  10  giant  axons  of  the  squid  (Loligo 
pealii),  estimated  from  frequency  of  conducted  impulses.  Stimulation  by  topical  application  of  in- 
dicated isotonic  solutions. 

The  relation  between  these  two  frequencies  is  illustrated  by  the  fol- 
lowing experiment,  which  makes  use  of  the  fact  that  a  polarizing  cur- 
rent may  modify  the  average  frequency  of  impulses  discharged  from  a 
calcium-deficient  region  of  nerve.  In  figure  13,  the  outward  flow  of 
current  across  the  chemically  altered  nerve  membrane  caused  a  tran- 
sient increase  of  the  average  impulse  frequency.  The  distribution  plot 
for  the  intervals  between  impulses  from  the  non-polarized  nerve  is 
shown  in  the  lower  half  of  figure  23.  The  intervals  between  some  im- 
pulses were  3.2  milliseconds;  other  impulses  recurred  at  intervals  which 
were  about  two  times  this  value. 

In  accordance  with  the  concepts  which  have  been  developed  in  this 
section,  we  may  say  that  there  was  a  rhythmic  excitatory  process,  in  the 
chemically  modified  portion  of  the  nerve,  of  a  fairly  constant  frequency. 


BRINK  AND  OTHERS:  CHEMICAL  EXCITATION  OF  NERVE    483 

50  r 

40 

30 


20 


10 


0  - 


1 


J I I I I I'll l_J 


8 


10 


u.     50 
O 


2     40 

CD 


30 

20 

10 

0 


J '      I      I I I I I I I I 


8 


10 


DURATION     OF     INTERVAL  (MS.) 

Figure  23.  The  average  number  of  impulses  per  second  in  a  chemically  excited  single  fiber  (frog) 
can  be  markedly  increased,  without  much  change  in  the  fundamental  penorl. 

Lower  graph :  Frequency  of  occurrence  in  a  train  of  impulses  of  intervals  having  values  indi- 
cite.l  en  I  lu-  abscis.-iie.  Stimulation  Uv  topical  app.ication  ol  a  fo.ution  containjig  sodium  citrate. 
The  average  number  of  conducted  impulses  per  second  was  about  220. 

Upper  graph  :  Similar  anals  sis  ol  a  consecutive  series  of  inipu..'-es  recorded  at  height  of  increa^^ed 
re^non^'e  evoked  hv  cathodal  po'arization  of  rhcmically  treated  region  (see  figures  12  and  13). 
The  average  number  per  second  of  conducted  impiilses  was  about  305. 

Some  of  these  cycles  of  local  change  failed  to  initiate  conducted  im- 
pulses, and,  accordingly,  some  of  the  intervals  between  successive  im- 
pulses were  integral  multiples  of  the  least  time  interval  of  3.2  milli- 
seconds.    The  average  impulse  frequency  was  220  per  second. 


484  ANNALS  NEW  YORK  ACADEMY  OF  SCIENCES 

A  similar  analysis  of  frequency  relations,  during  the  time  of  in- 
creased response  caused  by  the  cathodal  polarization,  was  then  made. 
The  distribution  of  intervals  between  impulses  is  plotted  in  the  upper 
part  of  FIGURE  23.  The  total  number  of  impulses  there  considered  is 
the  same  as  in  the  lower  plot.  During  this  time,  the  most  probable 
period  for  the  excitatory  process  was  only  slightly  changed,  but  the 
probability  that  impulses  would  recur  at  the  longer  intervals  was  then 
practically  zero.  The  action  of  the  polarizing  current  caused  more  im- 
pulses to  be  discharged  at  the  basic  interval,  and  this  increased  the  aver- 
age frequency  to  305  per  second.  In  general,  changes  in  this  average 
frequency  of  impulses,  caused  by  superimposed  chemical  or  physical 
agents,  occur  with  only  slight  modification  of  the  most  probable  least 
interval.  It  should  be  pointed  out,  however,  that,  under  certain  con- 
ditions of  intense  stimulation,  this  interval  can  be  decreased. 

Since  the  temporal  distribution  of  impulses  initiated  by  chemically 
excited  nerve  appears  to  be  regulated  by  a  local  rhythmic  process,  which 
is  determined  by  the  intrinsic  characteristics  of  the  nerve,  the  possibil- 
ity arises  that  the  same  mechanism  may  govern  the  discharge  of  im- 
pulses from  naturally  excited  sense  organs  and  motor  nerve  cells. 
Whether  the  mechanisms  described  above  do  have  such  a  general  sig- 
nificance, must  wait  upon  further  investigations. 

BIBLIOGRAPHY 

1.  Misske,  B. 

1930.     Biochem.  Zeit.  219:  320. 

2.  Lipton,  S.  R. 

1934.  Am.  J.  Physiol.  109:  457. 

3.  Blumenfeldt,  E. 

1925.  Biochem.  Zeit.  156:  236. 

4.  Gerard,  R.  W. 

1930.     Proc.  See.  Exp.  Biol.  &  Med.  27:  1052. 

5.  Brink,  F.,  T.  Sjostrand,  &  D.  W.  Bronk 
1939.     Am.  J.  Physiol.  126:  P442. 

6.  Adrian,  E.  D. 

1932.     The  Mechanism  of  Nervous  Action.     Univ.  of  Pennsylvania  Press. 

7.  Adrian,  E.  D.,  &  Y.  Zotterman 

1926.  J.  Physiol.  61:  151. 

8.  Adrian,  E.  D.,  &  D.  W.  Bronk 

1928.     J.  Physiol.  66:81. 

9.  Brink,  F.,  &  D.  W.  Bronk 

1937.     Proc.  Soc.  Exp.  Biol.  &  Med.  37:  94. 

10.  Chang,  T.  H.,  M.  Shaffer,  &  R.  W.  Gerard 

1935.  Am.  J.  Physiol.  Ill:  681. 

11.  Davies,  P.  W.,  &  F.  Brink 

1941.     Am.  J.  Physiol.  133:  P257. 

12.  Bronk,  D.  W.,  F.  Brink,  &  P.  W.  Davies 
1941.     Am.  J.  Physiol.  133:  P224. 


BRINK  AND  OTHERS:  CHEMICAL  EXCITATION  OF  NERVE    485 

13.  Adrian,  E.  D. 

1930.     Proc.  Roy.  Soc.  London  B  106:  596. 

14.  Erlanger,  J.,  &  E.  A.  Blair 

1935.  Am.  J.  Physiol.  114:  328. 

16.  Fessard,  A. 

1936.  Propri^t^s  Rythmiques  de  la  Matifere  Vivante.     I.  Nerfs    Myelinfeses. 
Hermann  &  Cie,  Paris. 

16.  Katz,  B. 

1936.     J.  Physiol.  88:  239. 

17.  Arvanitaki,  A. 

1939.     Arch.  Int.  de  Physiol.  49:  209. 

18.  Sjostrand,  T.,  F.  Brink,  &  D.  W.  Bronk 
1938.     Proc.  Soc.  Exp.  Biol.  &  Med.  38:  918. 

19.  Fessard,  A. 

1936.  Propri^tes  Rythmiques  de  la  Matiere  Vivante.     II.  Nerfs  Non   Mye- 
lineses.     Hermann  &  Cie,  Paris. 

20.  Brink,  F.,  T.  Sjostrand,  &  D.  W.  Bronk 
1938.     Am.  J.  Physiol.  123:  P22. 

21.  Cowan,  S.  L.,  &  W.  G.  Walter 

1938.  J.  Physiol.  91:  101. 

22.  Lorente  de  No,  R. 

1944.     J.  Cell.  &  Comp.  Physiol.  24:  85. 

23.  Bullock,  T.  H.,  D.  Nachmansohn,  &  M.  A.  Rothenberg 
1946.     J.  Neurophysiol.  9:  9. 

24.  Ranson,  S.  W.,  &  P.  R.  Billingsley 
1918.     J.  Comp.  Neurol.  29:  313. 

25.  Bronk,  D.  W. 

1939.  Symposium  on  the  Synapse.     C.  C.  Thomas,  Baltimore. 

26.  Feldberg,  W.,  &  A.  Vartiainen 
1935.    J.  Physiol.  83:  103. 

27.  Larrabee,  M.  G.,  &  D.  W.  Bronk 
Unpublished. 

28.  Bronk,  D.  W. 

1929.     J.  Physiol.  67:270. 

29.  Pumphrey,  R.  J. 

1935.     J.  Cell.  &  Comp.  Physiol.  6:  457. 

30.  Erlanger,  J. 

1937.  Electrical  Signs  of  Nervous  Activity.      University  of  Pennsylvania  Press, 
Philadelphia. 

31.  Hodgkin,  A.  L. 

1938.  Proc.  Roy.  Soc.  London  B  126:  87. 

32.  Brink,  F.,  &  D.  W.  Bronk 
1941.     Am.  J.  Physiol.  133:  P222. 

33.  Cole,  K.  S.,  &  R.  F.  Baker 
1940-1941.    J.  Gen.  Physiol.  24:  771. 

34.  Cole,  K.  S. 

1941-1942.    J.  Gen.  Physiol.  25:  29. 


ELECTRICAL  CHARACTERISTICS  OF 
ELECTRIC  TISSUE 

By  R.  T.  Cox,  C.  AV.  Coaxes,  and  M.  Vertner  Brown 

The  Department  oj  Physics,  The  Johns  Hopkins  University,  Baltimore,  Maryland; 

The   New    York  Aquarium,  New    York   Zoological  Society;   and   the 

Department  of  Physics,  College  of  the  City  of  New  York. 

The  group  of  electric  fishes  comprises  a  number  of  very  different 
varieties,  both  fresh  water  and  marine.  All  of  them  possess  special 
organs  capable  of  producing  transient  electric  discharges,  which,  in 
some  species,  are  quite  weak,  but  in  others,  are  powerful  enough  to  give 
a  severe  shock.  These  organs  vary  widely  among  the  different  species 
in  their  shape  and  size  and  in  their  position  and  orientation  in  the  body 
of  the  fish.  They  are  alike  in  having  a  common  unit  of  structure,  the 
electroplax. 

The  arrangement  of  the  electroplaxes  has  its  highest  geometrical  regu- 
larity in  the  electric  rays,  Torpedo  and  Narcine}'  ^  In  the  electric  or- 
gans of  these  genera,  they  are  piled  in  columns,  an  average  one  of 
which  contains  about  400  electroplaxes  in  Torpedo  marmorata  and  per- 
haps 300  in  Narcine  brasiliensis.  Each  column  extends  from  the  ven- 
tral to  the  dorsal  surface  of  the  body.  A  number  of  them,  side  by  side, 
form  each  of  the  two  electric  organs,  which  lie  in  the  disk-like  body  of 
the  fish  to  the  right  and  left  of  the  body  cavity,  just  outside  the  line  of 
gill  slits.  In  each  organ,  there  are  four  or  five  hundred  columns  in 
Torpedo  marmorata  and  Narcine  brasiliensis,  and  about  a  thousand  in 
T.  occidentalis.  During  the  discharge,  the  current  traverses  each  organ 
in  the  direction  from  its  ventral  to  its  dorsal  face.  Thus,  the  columns 
of  electroplaxes  discharge  in  parallel,  while,  within  each  column,  the 
electroplaxes  act  in  series  (figure  1). 

In  Torpedo  and  Narcine  alike,  the  electric  tissue  comprises  about  one 
sixth  of  the  whole  volume  of  the  fish.  In  the  electric  eel,  Electrophorus 
electricus,  it  makes,  by  contrast,  about  one  half.  Organs  of  such  a  size 
must  conform,  in  part,  to  the  shape  of  the  fish,  and  hence  there  cannot 
be  so  regular  an  arrangement  of  the  electroplaxes  as  in  the  rays.  It  is 
customary  to  distinguish  in  Electrophorus  three  pairs  of  organs:  the 
main  organs,  which  extend  along  the  posterior  four-fifths  of  the  length 
of  the  fish ;  the  much  smaller  organs  of  Hunter,  which  lie  under  the  main 
organs  along  their  entire  length;  and  the  organs  of  Sachs,  which  lie 

(487) 


488 


ANNALS  NEW  YORK  ACADEMY  OF  SCIENCER 


Figure    1.     Embryo    Narcine    brasUiensis,    dorsal    and    ventral    views.     Negative   faces   of    electric 
organs  at  e,  e.     (From  Zoologica.) 

a 

over  the  main  organs  in  the  posterior  half  of  the  fish.  The  organs  of 
Hunter  are  separated  from  the  main  organs  only  by  a  thin  layer  of 
muscle,  and  the  tissue  is  identical  in  adjacent  parts  of  the  two  pairs  of 
organs.  It  seems  more  reasonable  to  regard  the  organs  of  Hunter  as 
parts  of  the  main  organs  than  as  a  distinct  pair  (figure  2) . 


cox  AND  OTHERS:  ELECTRIC   TISSUE  489 

The  main  organs  are  of  nearly  uniform  cross-section  for  some  dis- 
tance from  their  anterior  end,  but  they  taper  toward  the  tail,  conform- 
ing to  the  ventral  surface  of  the  body  and,  in  the  posterior  portion,  to 
the  under  surface  of  the  organs  of  Sachs.  Although  this  tapering  pre- 
vents the  arrangement  of  electroplaxes  in  uniform  columns,  the  series- 
parallel  array  already  noted  in  the  rays  is,  nevertheless,  clearly  dis- 
cernible. In  Electrophorus,  the  axis  of  polarity  is  along  the  length  of 
the  fish.  Thus,  the  organs,  in  comparison  with  those  of  the  rays,  are 
very  much  elongated  along  the  line  of  series  connection  of  the  electro- 
plaxes. 


^m^^P^^0J 


'M"^ 


Drawing:   by    Ralph    Graeter 


Figure   2.     Electrophorus,   with   skin   removed   to  show   the  electric  organs. 
A,    main    organs;    B,    organs    of    Sachs,    overlapping    the    main    organs;    C,    organs    of    Hunter. 
(From  Bull.  N.  Y.  Zool.  Soc.) 

This  contrast  between  the  most  highly  specialized  electric  fishes  of 
the  fresh-water  and  marine  groups  has  a  significance  which  seems  to 
have  been  noticed  first  by  du  Bois-Reymond.^  The  combination  of  a 
fixed  number  of  electromotive  elements  to  supply  power  to  an  external 
circuit  of  given  resistance  is  a  well-known  problem  in  the  theory  of 
electric  networks.  The  solution  of  the  problem  shows  that  maximum 
power  will  be  delivered  to  the  external  circuit  by  the  combination  of 
the  elements  in  a  series-parallel  array,  such  that  the  resistance  of  the 
combination  is  equal  to  that  of  the  external  circuit.  Thus,  if  the 
external  resistance  is  high,  more  electromotive  elements  will  be  joined 
in  series;  if  it  is  low,  more  will  be  joined  in  parallel.  Fresh  water  has 
a  much  higher  specific  resistance  than  sea  water.  Consequently,  if  the 
condition  for  maximum  external  power  is  equally  approximated  in  the 
different  genera,  the  organs  of  the  fresh-water  fishes  will  be  elongated, 
and  those  of  the  marine  fishes  will  be  flattened,  along  the  axis  of  polar- 
ity. Most  of  the  varieties  confirm  such  a  generalization.  The  electric 
skates,  which  are  marine  fishes  with  weak  electric  organs  elongated  in 
the  direction  of  the  axis  of  polarity,  make  a  rather  puzzling  exception. 

The  arrangement  of  the  electroplaxes,  by  means  of  which  the  main 
organs  of  Electrophorus  are  accommodated  to  the  tapering  body  of  the 
fish,  is  simple  and  rather  interesting.^'  *  It  is  best  described  in  terms 
of  a  transverse  slice  just  thick  enough  to  contain  a  single  layer  of  elec- 
troplaxes.    Near  the  anterior  end  of  the  organs  of  a  fish  about  1  meter 


490  ANNALS  NEW  YORK  ACADEMY  OF  SCIENCES 

long,  the  thickness  of  this  single  electroplax  layer  is  about  .01  cm.  In 
fish  of  this  length,  the  cross-section  of  the  organs  near  the  anterior  end 
has  an  average  area  of  about  30  cm^.  Hence,  the  volume  of  the  single 
electropax  layer  is  0.3  cm-''.  The  organs  taper  caudally,  but,  as  the 
cross  section  decreases,  there  is  a  compensating  increase  in  the  thickness 
of  the  layer,  and  the  volume  is  nearly  uniform  over  most  of  the  length 
of  the  organs.  The  structure  is  much  as  if  uniform  layers  were  assem- 
bled in  a  long  column,  and  then  the  column  were  drawn  out  thin  toward 
one  end,  the  layers  being  changed  in  shape,  but  not  in  volume. 

These  long  organs  found  in  Electrophorus  offer  remarkable  advan- 
tages in  the  study  of  the  action  of  electric  tissue  in  the  living  fish.  The 
series  array  of  electroplax  layers  is  accessible  for  electrical  connection 
all  along  its  length,  rather  than  only  at  the  ends  as  in  the  rays.  The 
variation  in  structure  makes  it  possible  to  compare  in  the  same  speci- 
men the  electrical  characteristics  of  electroplax  layers  of  very  different 
dimensions.  Also,  Electrophorus,  which  comes  to  the  surface  to  breathe, 
can  be  kept  for  some  time  out  of  water  without  injury,  and  the  elec- 
trical characteristics  of  its  tissue  remain  constant  during  an  interval  in 
which  it  can  produce  a  thousand  or  more  electric  impulses. 

In  our  observations,  the  fish  is  removed  from  the  water  and  laid  in 
a  dry  wooden  trough.  Electrodes  made  of  aluminum  strip  1  cm.  wide 
may  be  placed  in  any  of  a  number  of  slots  in  this  trough.  These  make 
contact  with  the  skin  adjacent  to  the  electric  organs  and,  when  they 
are  connected  to  a  cathode-ray  oscillograph,  it  is  possible  to  record 
photographically  the  discharge  of  the  part  of  the  organs  included  be- 
tween the  electrodes  (figure  3).  The  measurements  made  with  the 
oscillograph  are  found  not  to  vary  appreciably  with  the  area  of  con- 
tact between  skin  and  electrode,  provided  this  area  is  not  less  than  a 
few  square  centimeters.  Of  course,  no  appreciable  dependence  on  the 
choice  of  a  metal  for  the  electrodes  is  to  be  expected,  since  the  voltages 
measured  are  very  much  greater  than  any  contact  potential  differences. 

When  the  electrodes  are  at  the  extremities  of  the  main  organs  of  a 
mature  specimen,  and  the  external  circuit  is  open,  so  that  there  is  no 
electric  current  outside  the  body  of  the  fish,  the  average  peak  voltage 
is  about  370  volts. ^  The  highest  voltage  we  have  measured  is  550. 
There  is  also  a  discharge  of  much  lower  voltage,  which  is  evidently  pro- 
duced by  the  organs  of  Sachs,  since  it  is  observed  only  when  some  part 
of  these  organs  lies  between  the  electrodes.  In  immature  specimens, 
the  voltages  are  smaller.  The  voltage  of  the  main  organs  increases 
with  their  length,  at  an  average  rate  of  8  volts  per  cm.,  until  the  organs 
attain  a  length  of  about  50  cm.     The  organs  may  ultimately  attain 


cox  AND  OTHERS:  ELECTRIC   TISSUE  491 

three  times  this  length,  but,  in  any  group  of  longer  specimens,  the  varia- 
tions in  voltage  appear  to  be  random. 

By  measuring  the  peak  voltage  between  electrodes  5  or  10  cm.  apart 
at  different  places  along  the  organs,  it  is  possible  to  compare  the  volt- 
age per  cm.  in  different  parts.  At  the  anterior  end,  where  the  number 
of  electroplax  layers  is  greatest,  the  voltage  per  cm.  is  also  greatest. 
It  decreases  caudally,  as  the  electroplax  layers  thicken.  The  voltage 
per  electroplax  layer  is  roughly  uniform  along  the  organs.     In  four 


T 


t » 


Figure  3.     Oscillographic  traces   of  the  discharge  of  Electrophorus: 

(a)  An   impulse    from    Sachs'   organs   followed   by   five  impulses   from   the  main  organs;    sweep 
period,  50  msec. 

(b)  Impulses    from    the    main    organs,    superimposed    by    successive    sweeps;     sweep    period,    4 
msec.     (From  Zoologica.) 

specimens,  values  from  0.11  to  0.16  volt  were  found  at  the  anterior  ends 
of  the  organs.  Somewhat  lower  values  are  found  in  the  posterior  parts, 
but,  over  most  of  the  length,  the  voltage  per  electroplax  layer  is  0.1 
volt  or  more.^'  ^  Values  around  0.1  volt  per  electroplax  layer  are  found 
also  in  Narcine  brasiliensis.  In  Narcine,  however,  and  also  in  Torpedo, 
the  voltage  of  the  organs  varies  widely  with  the  condition  of  the  fish. 
When  a  conductor  is  connected  between  the  electrodes,  so  that  the 
electric  tissue  produces  an  external  current,  the  peak  voltage  is  lower 
than  with  the  external  circuit  open.  If  care  is  taken  not  to  tire  the  fish, 
the  voltages  obtained  with  a  given  resistance  are  reproducible.  The 
resistance  R  of  the  external  conductor  being  known,  the  external  cur- 
rent /  is  found  from  the  measured  voltage  V,  by  the  relation,  /  =  V/R. 
When  conductors  of  successively  lower  resistance  are  employed,  the 
voltage  continues  to  decrease,  as  the  current  increases.     The  results  of 


492 


ANNALS  NEW  YORK  ACADEMY  OF  SCIENCES 


such  a  series  of  measurements  are  most  conveniently  shown  by  plotting 
the  values  of  the  voltage  against  the  values  of  the  current,  one  plotted 
point  representing  the  voltage  and  current  obtained  with  a  given  re- 
sistance (figure  4).     It  is  found  that  the  points  lie  near  a  straight  line 


lOO  . 


amperes 


Figure  4.    Peak  voltage  vs.  peak  current  in  external  circuits  of  different  resistance  joining  elec- 
trodes on  main  organs  of  Electrophorus.     (From  J.  Gen.  Physiol.) 

with  both  the  electric  eel  and  the  electric  rays.*  The  meaning  of  this 
result  is  that  the  electric  tissue,  at  least  at  the  peak  of  the  discharge 
and  within  the  uncertainty  of  the  measurements,  can  be  described  elec- 
trically in  terms  of  electromotive  force  and  ohmic  resistance. 

If  the  external  resistance  were  made  negligibly  small,  the  voltage 
also  would  be  negligible.  The  corresponding  current,  estimated  by 
extrapolating  the  straight  hne  of  the  graph  to  zero  voltage,  is  the  maxi- 
mum current  of  the  organs.  This  maximum  current  varies  from  one 
specimen  to  another  and,  in  Electrophorus,  it  varies  between  different 
parts  of  the  main  organs.  In  an  average  specimen,  around  1  m.  in 
length,  it  is  about  1  amp.  at  the  anterior  end  of  the  main  organs.  Values 
of  about  4  amp.  have  been  found  in  adult  specimens  of  Narcine 
brasiliensis,  and  a  value  of  120  amp.  was  roughly  estimated  in  a  single, 
very  large  specimen  of  Torpedo  occidentalis.^'  *•  ^ 

These  great  variations  are  due  more  to  differences  in  the  cross-sec- 
tions of  the  organs  than  to  differences  in  the  electrical  characteristics 
of  the  tissue.  The  maximum  current  per  unit  area  of  the  electroplax 
layer  was  found  to  have  values  in  Electrophorus  from  .02  to  .06  amp. 
per  cm.^,  the  average  being  about  .04.  In  Narcine  brasiliensis,  the 
value  is  about  0.1  and,  in  the  specimen  of  Torpedo  occidentalis  just 
mentioned,  it  was  about  0.2  amp.  per  cm.^     (In  this  calculation  and 


cox  AND  OTHERS:   ELECTRIC   TISSUE  493 

others  to  follow,  it  is  implied  that  the  two  paired  organs  discharge  simul- 
taneously. In  Narcine  and  Torpedo,  where  the  organs  are  far  apart, 
a  simple  experiment  shows  this  is  true.  The  evidence  in  respect  to 
Electrophoru.s  points  to  the  same  conclusion,  but  it  is  not  certain.) 

If  opening  the  external  circuit  prevented  any  current  in  the  organs, 
the  voltage  measured  with  the  circuit  open  would  be  equal  to  the  elec- 
tromotive force  of  the  part  of  the  organs  included  between  the  elec- 
trodes. Then,  the  maximum  voltage  per  electroplax  layer  would  be 
equal  to  the  electromotive  force  of  the  layer,  and  its  quotient  by  the 
maximum  current  per  cm.^  would  be  the  resistance  of  1  cm.-  of  the 
layer  at  the  peak  of  the  discharge.  However,  even  with  the  external 
circuit  open,  there  must  be  closed  circuits  within  the  body  of  the  fish, 
during  the  discharge.  Consequently,  the  voltage  per  electroplax  layer 
must  be  somewhat  less  than  the  electromotive  force  of  the  layer.  Its 
quotient  by  the  maximum  current  per  cm.-  is  still  of  some  significance 
as  a  lower  limit  for  the  resistance  of  1  cm.^  of  electroplax  layer.  It 
seems  likely,  also,  that  this  lower  limit  is  not  very  much  less  than  the 
actual  value.  In  Electrophonis,  minimum  values  thus  found  for  the 
resistance  of  1  cm.-  of  electroplax  layer  have  varied  in  different  speci- 
mens between  2  and  5  ohms.  In  two  specimens  of  Narcine  brasiliensis, 
the  values  were  about  1  ohm. 

It  is  interesting  that,  in  Electrophorus,  the  resistance  of  unit  area  of 
electroplax  layer  does  not  increase  caudally,  although  the  thickness  of 
the  layer  increases  about  ten-fold  from  the  anterior  to  the  posterior  end 
of  the  main  organs.  This  suggests  that  the  resistance  resides  prin- 
cipally at  boundaries  in  the  electric  tissue. 

From  the  observations  considered  thus  far,  it  is  seen  that  the  single 
electroplax  layer  in  Electrophorus  has  characteristic  electric  quantities 
which  are  roughly  uniform,  in  spite  of  wide  variations  in  the  arrange- 
ment of  the  electroplaxes  and  in  the  size  of  the  fish  observed.  Also, 
in  those  cases  in  which  comparison  has  been  possible,  it  is  found  that 
these  quantities  have  the  same  order  of  magnitude  in  Narcine  and 
Torpedo  as  in  Electrophorus. 

We  have  studied,  in  a  number  of  different  specimens  of  Electrophorus, 
the  variation  during  an  impulse  of  the  electrical  characteristics  of  the 
tissue.*  The  oscillograph  was  connected,  as  already  described,  to 
electrodes  placed  10  cm.  apart  against  the  main  organs  of  each  speci- 
men studied.  Impulses  with  the  external  circuit  open  and  closed, 
through  resistances  from  400  to  50  ohms,  were  recorded  photographic- 
ally. With  each  specimen  and  each  value  of  the  external  resistance, 
measurements  were  made  on  a  number  of  oscillographic  traces  at  each 


494 


ANNALS  NEW  YORK  ACADEMY  OF  SCIENCES 


of  several  short  time  intervals  after  the  beginning  of  the  impulse.  Val- 
u  s  of  the  voltage  measured  at  any  one  interval,  with  a  given  fish  and  a 
given  external  resistance,  were  then  averaged.  As  with  the  measure- 
ments at  the  peak  of  the  impulse,  the  results  are  conveniently  shown  by 
l)l()tting  the  voltage  against  the  external  current.  A  typical  set  of 
measurements  is  thus  shown  in  figure  5.     The  points  along  any  one 


-as 


O      ampefes         0.5 


1.0 


Figure  5.    Each  line  shows  voltage  vs.   current  at  one  instant,  in  external  circuits  of  different 
resistance  joining   electrodes  on   main   organs   of  Electro jhorus.     (From  J.    Gen.    Physiol.) 

line  show  values  of  voltage  and  current  obtained  with  different  external 
resistances,  at  the  same  interval  after  the  beginning  of  the  impulse. 
The  graph  farthest  to  the  top  and  right  shows  the  measurements  at  the 
peak  and,  thus,  corresponds  to  the  single  graph  shown  in  figure  4.  The 
other  two  graphs  show  the  measurements  at  two  later  instants  during 
the  impulse. 

Although  a  straight  line  cannot  be  drawn  precisely  through  the 
plotted  points  of  the  measurements  at  a  given  interval,  we  have  been 
unable  to  detect,  in  the  series  of  observations,  as  a  whole,  any  system- 
atic deviation  from  a  linear  relation.  It  appears,  therefore,  that  the 
tissue  can  be  described  electrically  in  terms  of  electromotive  force 
and  ohmic  resistance,  not  only  at  the  peak  of  the  impulse,  but  at  other 
times  as  well. 

A  simple  diagram  for  such  a  description  is  shown  in  figure  6.  In 
reference  to  this  figure,  let  E  denote  the  electromotive  force  of  the  part 
of  the  organs  included  between  the  electrodes  at  p  and  q,  and  let  r  de- 


cox  AND  OTHERS:  ELECTRIC   TISSUE 


495 


note  the  internal  resistance.  The  current  in  whatever  circuits  are 
closed  within  the  body  of  the  fish  is  treated,  somewhat  arbitrarily,  as 
traversing  a  single  path  of  resistance,  R\ 


R' 


t 


i. 


fA/^W 


LVWvVWJ 

R 

Figure  6.    Simple  diagram  for  describing  the  impulse  from  the  main  organs  of  Electrophorus. 
(From  J.  Gen.  Physiol.) 

Let  I,  I,  and  /'  denote  the  currents  in  r,  R,  and  R',  respectively.  Since 
the  current  i  branches  to  form  the  currents  /  and  /',  it  follows  that: 

i  =  i  +  r. 

Let  V  denote  the  voltage  measured  by  the  oscillograph,  connected  at 
p  and  q.  This  voltage  may  be  reckoned  in  any  of  the  three  branches 
of  the  network,  and,  thus,  we  obtain  three  expressions  for  V,  as  follows: 
V  =  IR,  V  =  I'R',  V  =  E  -  ir. 

Eliminating  i  and  /',  among  these  four  equations,  we  obtain  two  ex- 
pressions for  the  external  current  /  in  terms  of  the  voltage  V: 


/  = 


I  =  --  V 


R  '        r        '  \r    '   R' 

The  first  of  these  equations  is  used  to  find  the  current  /  from  the 
measured  voltage  V,  by  means  of  the  known  resistance  R.  The  values 
of  /  and  V  are  plotted  as  in  figure  5.  The  other  equation  is  then  used 
to  interpret  the  graph  so  obtained.  If  the  resistances  r  and  R'  are 
ohmic,  they  are  constants  in  this  equation,  which  is  then  a  linear  rela»- 
tion  between  /  and  V,  such  as  is  actually  found  to  exist.  If,  in  this 
equation,  we  let  V  =  E,  we  find  the  corresponding  value  of  /  to  be 
—E/R\  If  we  suppose  that  E  and  R'  have  the  same  values  at  dif- 
ferent instants  during  the  discharge,  this  equation  states  that  the 
graphs  for  the  different  instants,  when  extrapolated  to  negative  values 
of  /,  will  intersect  at  a  point.  The  co-ordinates  of  this  point,  moreover, 
will  determine  the  values  of  E  and  R' . 

Actually,  the  lines  shown  in  the  figure,  which  are  typical  of  those 
obtained  from  measurements  after  the  peak  of  the  impulse  has  been 
attained,  do  nearly  meet  in  a  point.     The  fact  that  the  graphs  of  the 


496  ANNALS  NEW  YORK  ACADEMY  OF  SCIENCES 

measurements  at  later  instants  are  steeper  than  the  graph  of  the  peak 
values,  indicates  a  rise  in  the  internal  resistance  r,  after  the  peak  is 
passed. 

These  results  led  us,  earlier,  to  suppose  that  the  electromotive  force 
might  be  constant  throughout  the  impulse  and  between  impulses  as 
well,  the  discharge  being  caused  by  a  transient  drop  in  the  resistance 
from  a  very  high  resting  value.'  At  that  time,  we  had  not  succeeded  in 
plotting  graphs  of  voltage  and  current  at  instants  during  the  brief 
interval  of  rising  voltage.  Both  the  steepness  of  the  rising  phase  of 
the  oscillographic  trace  and  its  consequent  faintness  in  the  photo- 
graphs made  measurement  difficult  in  this  interval.  Measurements 
which  we  have  made  more  i-ecently  iiave  obliged  us  to  reconsider  our 
earlier  opinion.  The  graphs  of  voltage  and  current  obtained  from 
measurements  during  the  interval  of  rising  voltage  do  not  meet  at  a 
point.  Moreover,  even  during  the  interval  of  falling  voltage,  we  find 
that  deviations,  which  were  formerly  within  our  estimated  errors  of 
measurement  and  which  we,  therefore,  supposed  were  accidental,  ap- 
pear consistently  in  the  later  observations. 

The  variation  in  resistance  during  the  interval  of  falling  voltage 
seems,  in  any  case,  well  established.  It  seems  probable,  also,  that  the 
electromotive  force  is  at  least  approximately  constant  during  this  phase. 
Our  immediate  object  is  an  estimate  of  the  total  electric  energy  pro- 
duced in  an  impulse.  Fortunately  for  this  purpose,  the  time  after 
the  attainment  of  peak  voltage  is  most  of  the  duration  of  the  impulse. 
Although  the  changes  in  the  electrical  characteristics,  during  the  brief 
phase  of  rapidly  rising  voltage,  remain  uncertain,  the  assumptions  made 
about  them  in  the  calculation  of  the  energy  can  be  varied  widely,  with- 
out changing  the  result  by  more  than  about  10  per  cent. 

From  the  equations  already  given,  it  follows  that  the  current  i 
traversing  the  electric  tissue  is  related  to  the  current  /,  measured  in 
the  external  circuit  by  the  equation : 

7  =  (1  +  R/R/)r. 

In  this  equation,  E  is  known,  and  R'  is  determined  by  the  intersection 
of  the  voltage-current  graphs.  Thus,  the  current  in  the  electric  tissue 
is  found. 

The  charge  q  which  passes  through  the  tissue  in  one  impulse  is 
given  by : 

q  =  jidt, 

where  t  denotes  the  time,  and  the  integration  is  performed  over  the  dur- 
ation of  the  impulse.     The  integration  can  easily  be  done  graphically. 


cox  AND  OTHERS:  ELECTRIC   TISSUE  497 

The  charge  passing  through  1  cm.^  of  electroplax  layer  is  found  by 
dividing  q  by  the  cross-sectional  area  of  the  electric  organs.  (The 
measurements  were  made  at  the  anterior  end,  where  the  cross-section 
is  nearly  uniform.)  Since  the  fish  on  which  the  measurements  were 
made  were  not  killed,  the  cross-section  had  to  be  determined  indirectly 
from  external  measurements.  Two  methods  were  employed.  In  the 
first  method,  the  girth  of  the  fish  was  measured.  The  cross-section  of 
the  organs  was  then  estimated  by  comparison  with  measurements  on 
dissected  fish,  on  the  assumption  that  the  cross-section  of  the  organs 
bears  a  constant  ratio  to  the  square  of  the  girth.  In  the  second 
method,  a  simple  mechanical  device  was  employed  to  trace  the  outline 
of  the  cross-section  of  the  live  fish.  The  area  enclosed  by  this  outline 
was  measured,  and  the  cross-section  of  the  organs  was  taken  as  59  per 
cent  of  the  total  area,  this  percentage  having  been  obtained  from  meas- 
urements on  a  number  of  dissected  specimens.  When  both  methods 
were  used,  the  agreement  between  the  results  was  fairly  good,  the 
values  determined  by  the  two  methods  showing  a  mean  deviation  of 
around  eight  per  cent. 

(However,  the  use  of  only  the  first  method,  in  another  experiment, 
led  to  a  rather  serious  error."  The  number  used  then  as  the  ratio  of 
the  cross-sectional  area  of  the  organs  to  the  square  of  the  girth  of  the 
fish  was  obtained  from  measurements  on  only  two  specimens.  Also, 
the  girth  of  these  sections  was  not  measured  in  the  same  way  as  on  the 
live  fish,  and  this  led  to  a  further  discrepancy,  which  was  increased 
when  the  girth  was  squared.  The  correction  of  the  resulting  error  to 
accord  with  our  new  measurements  on  a  larger  number  of  specimens 
requires  that  the  values  of  electric  energy  per  gm.  and  impulse  given 
in  the  paper  referred  to  should  be  reduced  about  40  per  cent.  Instead 
of  inferring,  as  we  did  in  a  subsequent  paper,^  that  the  total  electric 
energy  is  about  equal  to  that  of  the  breakdown  of  phosphocreatine  and 
the  production  of  lactic  acid,  we  should  now  infer  that  the  electric 
energy  is  about  six  tenths  of  the  sum  of  the  energies  of  these  two  chem- 
ical processes.  The  correction  brings  this  result  into  fair  agreement 
with  that  reported  by  Nachmansohn,  elsewhere  in  this  volume.) 

The  electromotive  force  of  the  part  of  the  organs  included  between 
the  electrodes  was  determined  by  the  point  of  intersection  of  the  volt- 
age-current graphs.  This  quantity  was  divided  by  the  distance  be- 
tween the  elctrodes,  to  give  the  electromotive  force  per  cm.  along  the 
column  of  electroplax  layers.  The  product  of  the  electromotive  force 
per  cm.,  regarded  as  constant  during  the  impulse,  by  the  charge  travers- 
ing one  cm.^  of  electroplax  layer,  is  the  total  electric  energy  per  cm.^ 
produced  in  one  impulse. 


498  ANNALS  NEW  YORK  ACADEMY  OF  SCIENCES 

The  charge  passing  through  the  organs,  and  hence,  also,  the  energy, 
depend  on  the  resistance  of  the  external  circuit,  as  well  as  on  the  elec- 
trical characteristics  of  the  tissue.  For  comparison  with  the  chemical 
measurements,  it  is,  of  course,  essential  that  the  external  resistance 
should  be  the  same  in  the  electrical,  as  in  the  chemical,  experiments. 
Otherwise,  the  choice  of  an  external  resistance  is,  within  limits,  un- 
important. The  fish  on  which  the  electrical  measurements  have  been 
made  form  three  groups  according  to  size,  with  average  lengths  of 
67,  103,  and  180  cm.  We  have  used  an  external  resistance  of  200  ohms 
with  the  two  groups  of  smaller  length,  and  of  100  ohms  with  the  other 
group.  These  resistances  were  roughly  the  same  as  the  internal  re- 
sistances, averaged  over  the  time  of  the  impulse,  of  the  part  of  the  or- 
gans between  the  electrodes.  Consequently,  the  condition  of  the  ex- 
periment approximated  the  requirement  for  maximum  energy  in  the 
external  circuit. 

The  results  are  summarized  in  table  1,  which  shows,  for  each  speci- 
men and  for  the  average  of  each  group,  the  electromotive  force  per  cm., 
the  charge  passing  in  one  impulse  through  1  cm.^  of  electroplax  layer, 
and  the  total  electric  energy  produced  in  one  impulse  in  1  cm.^  of  elec- 
tric tissue.  With  the  electrical  units  employed,  the  product  of  the  elec- 
tromotive force  per  cm.  by  the  charge  per  cm.^  would  give  the  energy 
per  cm.^  in  microjoules.  For  more  convenient  comparison  with  the 
chemical  energies  reported  by  Nachmansohn,  the  energies  have  been 
given,  instead,  in  microcalories.  Since  the  tissue  has  nearly  unit  spe- 
cific gravity,  the  energy  per  cm.^  may  be  taken  as  the  energy  per  gm., 
without  serious  error. 

It  has  already  been  mentioned  that  the  assumption  made  in  the  cal- 
culations, that  the  right  and  left  organs  discharge  simultaneously, 
though  probable,  is  still  unproved.  If  the  fact  should  be  that  the  or- 
gans discharge  separately,  then  the  given  values  of  the  charge  per  cm.^ 
and  the  energy  per  cm.^  would  have  to  be  doubled.  The  same  correc- 
tion would  have  to  be  applied  to  the  values  of  the  chemical  energy,  and, 
therefore,  the  comparison  of  the  electrical  and  chemical  energies  made 
by  Nachmansohn  is  valid  in  either  case. 

The  individual  values  of  electromotive  force  per  cm.  show  a  mean 
deviation  of  12  per  cent  from  the  average  of  the  group.  The  mean 
deviation  of  the  charge  per  cm.^  is  15  per  cent,  and  that  of  the  energy 
per  cm.^  is  23  per  cent.  The  risk  of  a  serious  uncertainty  in  the  aver- 
ages, beyond  that  indicated  by  these  deviations,  depends  on  the  possi- 
bility that  the  equations  used  in  the  computation  are  seriously  in  error. 
The  evidence  by  which  they  were  justified  has  already  been  given. 


cox  AND  OTHERS:  ELECTRIC   TISSUE 

Table  1 
Electrical  Measurements  on  Eleclrophorus 


499 


Length 

of 

fish 

(cm.) 

Cross- 
section 

of 

electric 

organs 

(cm.2) 

Electro- 
motive 
force 

per  cm. 
(volts) 

Charge 
per  cm. 2 

and 
impulse 
(micro- 
coulombs) 

Energy 
per  cm.' 

and 
impulse 
(micro- 
calories) 

External  resistance, 
200  ohms 

60 
62 
65 
68 
68 
70 
72 
74 

10 
12 
13 
10 
16 
14 
13 
13 

20 
15 
15 
19 
23 
20 
21 
20 

11 
20 
18 
22 
21 
20 
21 
19 

55 

72 
64 
99 

118 
95 

108 
89 

Ave. 

67 

13 

19 

19 

88 

E.xternal  resistance, 
200  ohms 

90 
93 
96 
100 
102 
104 
107 
112 
121 

28 
33 
23 
32 
38 
31 
31 
36 
27 

16 
13 
13 
18 
14 
12 
18 
12 
15 

18 
12 
14 
14 
12 
10 
13 
11 
16 

71 
35 
43 
59 
40 
29 
54 
32 
57 

Ave. 

103 

31 

15 

13 

47 

External  resistance, 
100  ohms 

164 
169 
186 

187 
187 
188 

67 
70 
70 
67 
72 
66 

15 
12 
11 
10 
13 
10 

5.0 
9.5 
8.0 
8.7 
11.3 
10.8 

18 
27 
21 
20 
35 
26 

Ave. 

183 

69 

12 

8.9 

24 

It  seems  unlikely  that  the  actual  values  of  the  electrical  quantities 
should  be  much  lower  than  those  given  in  the  table.  The  voltage  meas- 
ured with  the  external  circuit  open  sets  a  lower  limit  to  the  possible 
value  of  the  electromotive  force.  The  maximum  measured  voltage  is, 
on  the  average  for  all  the  specimens,  77  per  cent  of  the  value  found  for 
the  electromotive  force.  It  seems  reasonable  that  the  current  in  cir- 
cuits closed  in  the  body  of  the  fish  should  cause  a  voltage  drop  of  23 
per  cent.  Similarly,  the  charge  passing  through  the  external  resist- 
ance, which  is  obtained  directly  from  simple  oscillographic  measure- 
ments, sets  a  lower  limit  to  the  possible  magnitude  of  the  charge  tra- 


500  ANNALS  NEW  YORK  ACADEMY  OF  SCIENCES 

versing  the  tissue.  This  lower  limit  is,  on  the  average,  82  per  cent  of 
the  computed  total  charge.  It  seems  unlikely  that  the  external  charge 
is,  actually,  much  nearer  than  this  to  the  total  charge.  From  these 
considerations,  we  should  judge  it  improbable  that  our  assumptions 
involve  a  systematic  error,  whereby  the  actual  values  of  the  energy 
should  be  consistently  more  than  about  15  per  cent  lower  than  those 
found  by  the  method  we  have  used. 

The  question  as  to  how  much  the  actual  values  may  exceed  those  we 
find,  presents  a  greater  uncertainty.  Although,  as  was  just  explained, 
it  is  difficult  to  propose  an  equivalent  network  for  the  electric  organs 
which  will  dissipate  much  less  energy  than  we  allow  for  in  internal 
currents,  there  is  no  difficulty  in  proposing  a  network  to  dissipate  more 
energy.  This  follows  from  Thevenin's  theorem  of  electric  networks, 
according  to  which  any  electromotive  force  inferred  from  external 
measurements  may  be  regarded,  alternatively,  as  the  open-circuit  volt- 
age of  a  concealed  network  containing  a  higher  electromotive  force. 
On  the  other  hand,  our  calculations  ascribe  to  the  single  electroplax 
layer  an  electromotive  force  about  as  high  as  any  that  are  found  at 
boundaries  in  bioelectric  phenomena.  This  suggests  that  the  actual 
values  of  the  electrical  quantities  are  not  very  much  greater  than  those 
we  calculate.  For  the  energy,  which  is  the  most  uncertain  of  these 
quantities,  twice  the  calculated  value  appears  to  be  a  safe  upper  limit. 

REFERENCES 

1.  Gotch,  F. 

1900.  The  Physiology  of  Electrical  Organs.  Textbook  of  Physiology,  Vol.  2. 
Young  J.  Pentland,  Edinburgh  and  London;  MacMillan,  New  York. 
(This  article  contains  references  to  a  number  of  original  sources.) 

2.  Cox,  R.  T.,  &  C.  M.  Breder,  Jr. 

1943.  Observations  on  the  electric  discharge  of  Narcine  brasiliensis  (Olfers). 
Zoologica  28:  45. 

3.  Cox,  R.  T.,  W.  A.  Rosenblith,  J.  A.  Cutler,  R.  S.  Mathews,  &  C.  W.  Coates 

1940.  A  comparison  of  some  electrical  and  anatomical  characteristics  of  the 
electric  eel,  Electrophorus  electricus  (Linnaeus).     Zoologica  25:  553. 

4.  Cox,  R.  T.,  C.  W.  Coates,  &  M.  V.  Brown 

1945  Relations  between  the  structure,  electrical  characteristics,  and  chemical 
processes  of  electric  tissue.     J.  Gen.  Physiol.  28:  187. 

5.  Coates,  C.  W.,  &  R.  T.  Cox 

1945.  A  comparison  of  length  and  voltage  in  the  electric  eel,  Electrophorus 
electricus  (Linnaeus).     Zoologica.  30:  89. 

6.  Coates,  C.  W.,  &  R.  T.  Cox 

1942.  01)servations  on  the  electric  discharge  of  Torpedo  occidentalis.  Zoologica. 
27:25. 

7.  Nachmansohn,  D.,  R.  T.  Cox,  C.  W.  Coates,  &  A.  L.  Machado 

1943.  Action  potential  and  enzyme  activity  in  the  electric  organ  oi  Electrophorus 
electricus.  II.  Phosphocreatine  as  energy  source  of  the  action  potential. 
J.  Neurophysiol.  6:  383. 


SOME  BASIC  ASPECTS  OF  THE  ACTIVITY 
OF  ELECTRIC  PLATES 

By  Alfred  Fessard 
Institut  Marey,  Paris,  France 

The  most  conspicuous  manifestations  of  the  activity  of  electric  or- 
gans are:  (a)  the  electrical  discharge  itself;  (b)  variations  of  the  elec- 
trical conductivity  of  the  tissue;  (c)  thermal  effects;  (d)  chemical 
(metabolic)  transformations. 

Every  individual  feature  of  the  total  discharge  is  more  or  less  closely 
associated  with  the  species  of  fish  examined,  with  its  shape  and  dimen- 
sions, and  with  the  structural  arrangement  of  the  physiological  units 
(or  electric  plates)  composing  the  organ.  However,  on  the  cellular 
dimension  scale,  the  behavior  of  these  plates  shows  a  striking  unity, 
and  is  generally  considered  as  an  ordinary  bioelectric  phenomenon. 
The  classical  conceptions  of  nerve  physiology  are  applicable  here,  and 
these,  combined  with  our  rapidly  progressing  knowledge  of  neuromus- 
cular transmission  mechanisms,  have  been  used  for  explaining  the  pro- 
duction of  electricity  in  these  organs.  Here,  briefly  exposed  for  each 
essential  manifestation  of  activity,  are  the  most  significant  facts  in 
favor  of  the  unitary  conception,  together  with  some  details  concerning 
our  experimental  contribution  to  this  problem. 

A.  The  most  recent  determinations  of  the  electromotive  force  per 
plate  show,  on  Electrophorus  electricus,  a  fairly  uniform  value:  viz., 
0.14  volts  (Coates  and  Cox^^).  This  is  in  agreement  with  the  highest 
value  found  by  Curtis  and  Cole^*'  for  the  action  potential  from  the 
squid  giant  axon.  As  for  the  time  course  of  the  discharge,  non-typical 
shapes  are  often  observed  (Cox,  Coates,  and  Brown^^),  which  may 
wrongly  be  considered  as  representing  the  elementary  process.  Our 
own  research  in  this  field  has  definitely  convinced  us  that  these  non- 
typical  wave  shapes  are  due  to  statistical  dispersion  effects,  the  causes 
of  which  are  low  velocity  at  the  periphery  and  differences  in  length  of 
winding  nerve  endings. 

In  our  experiments  (most  of  them  still  unpublished),  we  used  organs 
of  Torpedo  marmorata  and  of  Raia  undulata.  Our  purpose  was  to 
record  the  discharge  of  a  single  plate  and  then  to  interpret  the  complex 
discharges  in  terms  of  their  components.     Columns  of  electric  tissue 

(501) 


I 


502  ANNALS  NEW  YORK  ACADEMY  OF  SCIENCES 

were  carefully  isolated,  and  transverse  slices  about  1  millimeter  thick 
cut  out  with  fine  scissors.  Such  fragments  contain  10  to  20  plates, 
many  of  them  damaged  by  the  dissection.  The  intact  ones  retain  a 
good  excitability  for  hours.  By  transverse  electrical  stimulation  at 
threshold  values,  it  is  sometimes  possible  to  record  responses  which,  be- 
yond all  doubt,  are  elementary  (plate  4A,  lower  record).  They  obey 
the  all-or-nothing  law  and  are  the  conspicuous  components  of  the  com- 
plex waves  obtained  with  slightly  higher  intensities.  In  Torpedo,  they 
last  1.75-2.25  msec,  (rising  phase,  0.5-0.7  msec.) ;  in  skates  (Auger  and 
Fessard^),  they  are  much  longer,  about  12  msec,  (rising  phase,  2-2.5 
msec).  Now  the  discharge  of  a  whole  column  is  noticeably  of  longer 
duration  (plate  4A,  middle  record).  In  spite  of  the  shortness  of  the 
nervous  tracts  contained  in  these  small  pieces  of  tissue,  latencies  vary, 
from  less  than  1  msec,  to  more  than  4  msec,  and  dispersion  of  com- 
ponents lengthens  the  wave  duration  to  2  or  3  times  its  elementary 
value.  The  natural  discharge  starting  off  the  whole  organ  is  hardly 
longer.  The  long  distance  command  is  transmitted  by  high  velocity 
fibers  of  large  diameter,  and  these  do  not  introduce  such  an  important 
shift  in  components  as  do  the  thin  nerve  branches  at  the  periphery. 
The  elementary  electrical  process  is,  therefore,  comparable  to  a  nerve 
action  potential,  never  being  diphasic,  as  conduction  is  absent.  Posi- 
tive after-potentials  have  never  been  observed.  Mention  must  be  made 
of  some  variations  in  the  declining  phase,  which  is  sometimes  longer 
than  usual,  especially  in  skates.  In  these  cases,  the  discharge  is  more 
like  an  end-plate  potential  than  an  action  potential. 

B.  The  analogy  between  the  electric  organs  and  other  excitable 
systems  retains  its  value  when  the  electrical  conductivity  is  studied 
during  the  discharge.  Using  the  impedance  bridge  method,  applied  by 
Cole  and  Curtis^^  to  the  squid  giant  axon,  we  have  observed^  a  transient 
drop  of  impedance  during  the  activity  of  Torpedo  and  Raia  organs. 
Recently,  Cox,  Coates,  and  Brown^^  have  obtained  indirect  evidence 
of  a  diminution  in  ohmic  resistance  during  the  discharge  of  Electroph- 
orus  electricus. 

Plate  4B  shows  one  of  our  records  obtained  with  a  double  beam 
cathode  ray  oscillograph.  The  upper  record  is  the  discharge  of  an 
isolated  column  {Raia),  placed  in  one  branch  of  an  impedance  bridge; 
the  lower  one  shows  the  reappearance  of  the  15,000  cycles  oscillation 
feeding  the  bridge.  The  two  phenomena  start  simultaneously,  instead 
of  showing  a  shift  as  in  the  nerve,  a  fact  easily  interpreted  as  a  lack  of 
conduction     The  impedance  change  is  slight,  less  than  10  per  cent,  but 


FESSARD:  ACTIVITY  OF  ELECTRIC  PLATES  503 

here  the  presence  of  inactive  tissue  around  the  plates  renders  illusory 
any  attempt  to  introduce  quantitative  measurements. 

C.  In  observations  unfortunately  never  repeated,  Bernstein  and 
Tschermak^"  have  shown  an  initial  cooling  of  the  organ,  when  the  dis- 
charge was  externally  derived  through  a  resistance.  They  interpreted 
this  fact  as  being  inconsistent  with  a  chemical  mechanism  of  the  energy 
supply.  Meyerhof-"  criticized  this  interpretation,  without,  however, 
denying  the  fact,  which  should  be  reinvestigated  with  the  more  per- 
fected methods  now  available. 

D.  Recent  investigations  into  the  biochemistry  of  electric  organs 
proved  their  metabolism  to  be  quite  similar  to  that  of  nerve  tissue  or 
striated  muscle.  Cholinesterase  has  been  shown  to  be  very  abundant 
in  electric  tissue  (concerning  this  significant  presence  of  cholinesterase 
in  electric  organs,  as  at  all  neuronal  surfaces,  see  Nachmansohn^"). 
This  fact  implies  that  acetylcholine  plays  an  essential  role  here,  as 
elsewhere. 

It  was  under  the  impetus  of  Nachmansohn  that  research  was  under- 
taken by  Feldberg,  Fessard,  and  Nachmansohn  to  detect  the  prob- 
able presence  of  acetylcholine  in  electric  organs,  and  to  study  the  part 
it  plays  in  the  production  of  the  discharge.  In  these  experiments,  posi- 
tive results  were  obtained  in  Torpedo  organs.  During  stimulation,  the 
ester  appears  in  the  perfusate,  and  arterial  injections  of  micro-doses  of 
acetylcholine  produce  long-lasting  electrical  changes. ^^'  ^® 

The  next  step  was  when  Nachmansohn  and  his  collaborators"  dem- 
onstrated that  cholinesterase  is  localized  at  the  active  surfaces  in  elec- 
tric organs,  its  concentration  being  strictly  correlated  with  the  maxi- 
mum voltage  and,  consequently,  like  the  electromotive  force,  with  the 
number  of  plates  per  unit  length.  Now  the  acetylcholine  release  is 
able  to  start  the  chain  of  reactions,  beginning  with  the  phosphorylated 
compounds  acting  in  nerve  or  muscle  metabolism,  which  we  now  know 
to  be  present,  together  with  the  associated  enzymatic  system,  in  the 
electric  organs  (Baldwin  and  Needham,*  Kisch,^^  Nachmansohn  et 
al.^^).  The  research  in  this  field  has  now  reached  a  quantitative  as- 
pect, and  the  energy  liberated  by  the  breakdown  of  phosphocreatine 
and  the  formation  of  lactic  acid  during  the  discharge  can  be  compared 
with  the  electrical  energy  released  (Nachmansohn  et  oL'^) . 

In  concluding  this  short  survey,  we  can  say  that  there  is  a  striking 
convergence  of  data,  allowing  the  electric  plate  to  be  put  side-by-side 
with  the  nerve  and  muscle  units,  from  the  point  of  view  of  their  electro- 


504  ANNALS  NEW  YORK  ACADEMY  OF  SCIENCES 

chemical  properties.  However,  some  uncertainties  persist  as  to  the 
particular  mode  of  production  of  their  electrical  discharges.  We  are 
still  ignorant  of  the  way  in  which  acetylcholine  may  act  at  an  inter- 
face to  generate  electricity.  This  ignorance  is  general,  but  we  do  not 
even  know,  in  electric  organs,  where  this  active  interface  lies;  and,  to 
assign  a  definite  physiological  significance  to  the  plate,  we  are  faced 
with  at  least  three  different  views.  Although  we  are  far  from  being 
able  to  give  a  satisfactory  answer  to  these  three  debated  questions,  we 
shall  briefly  discuss  the  last  two  in  the  light  of  the  experimental  evi- 
dence we  have  obtained,  up  to  the  present,  in  our  research  on  the 
Torpedo. 

I.   PHYSIOLOGICAL   SIGNIFICANCE  OF   THE  ELECTRIC 

PLATE 

A.  According  to  a  current  view,  the  electric  plate  is  considered  as  an 
element  of  a  true  effector,  and  this  implies  the  notion  of  its  physiological 
individuality.  As  in  the  case  of  the  muscle,  this  special  effector  would 
be  normally  set  into  activity  through  a  "relay"  mechanism,  and  should 
show,  by  direct  stimulation,  an  excitability  of  its  own.  This  com- 
parison with  striated  muscle  is  all  the  more  justified,  as  both  effectors 
have  a  common  embryonic  origin  (the  Malapterurus  organ  excepted). 
On  the  other  hand,  functional  analogies  seem  to  exist  between  the 
electrical  discharge  and  the  muscular  contraction  (Marey^^) . 

B.  However,  the  regression  of  all  vestiges  of  striation  in  the  adult 
stage  of  the  more  powerful  electric  organs;  the  absence  of  myosin  (re- 
placed by  mucin^)  among  the  proteins  of  electrical  tissues;  and,  above 
all,  the  simultaneous  disappearance  of  direct  and  indirect  excitability, 
under  different  conditions  (nerve  degeneration,  fatigue,  cooling)  have 
thrown  serious  doubts  upon  the  value  of  the  analogy.  Some  authors 
have  gone  so  far  as  to  consider  the  possibility  of  a  purely  nervous  ori- 
gin of  the  discharge.  Gotch^^  wrote  that  "the  excitatory  electromotive 
change  may  be  nothing  more  than  the  fact  that  when  an  excitatory 
process  travels  down  a  nerve,  the  nerve  trunk  becomes  negative  to  its 
terminal  cross-section."  The  maximum  value  of  a  nerve  action  po- 
tential is  the  same  as  that  of  an  electric  plate,  and  it  is  suggestive  to 
note  that  the  elementary  plate  discharge  and  the  single  fiber  action 
potential  of  the  nerve  commanding  the  organ  have  exactly  the  same 
duration  (plate  4A,  upper  part) .  Furthermore,  the  chemical  data  are 
far  from  being  opposed  to  this  conception,  which  tends  to  reduce  the 
role  of  the  plate  to  that  of  a  simple  support  for  a  richly  expanding 
nervous  branching. 


FESSARD:  ACTIVITY  OF  ELECTRIC  PLATES  505 

C.  Nevertheless,  as  Rosenberg  points  out  in  his  Review  on  the  sub- 
ject,^'^  it  seems  unlikely  that  the  plate  has  only  this  passive  role  of 
support.  According  to  our  views,  the  main  difficulty  of  a  purely  nervous 
theory  lies  in  the  fact  that  the  orientation  of  the  discharge  does  not 
agree  with  the  symmetries  offered  by  the  nerve  distribution. 

An  electric  organ  is  sometimes  described  as  an  accumulation  of  modi- 
fied motor  end-plates.  This  view  is  more  in  agreement  than  any  other 
with  the  ontogenic  facts  and  with  the  analogies  suggested  by  histology. 
There  are  many  nuclei  in  the  plate  near  the  innervated  face,  as  in  the 
sarcoplasmic  sole  of  the  striated  muscle.  Couteaux  recently  described, 
at  the  myoneural  junction,  a  rod-like  structure  which  is  strikingly 
analogous  to  that  long  believed  to  be  specific  of  electric  organs.^* 

The  existence,  now  well  established,  of  a  localized  response  at  the 
nerve-muscle  junction  (end-plate  potential,  e.p.p.),  preceding  the 
muscle  fiber  propagated  impulse,  renders  the  analogy  still  more  evi- 
dent. This  e.p.p.,  like  the  discharge,  is  accompanied  by  an  impedance 
change  that  follows  the  same  time-course  as  that  observed  in  electric 
organs  (Katz^*) :  i.e.,  a  non-delayed  rising  phase  and  a  maximum  ef- 
fect near  the  inflexion  point  of  the  potential  variation. 

Our  experiments  on  small  isolated  fragments  contribute  to  show  that 
Hypothesis  A  cannot  be  retained,  as  it  is  really  impossible  to  isolate  the 
plate  as  a  functional  effector  unit.  They  are  also  more  in  agreement 
with  Hypothesis  C  than  with  B. 

a.  AVe  thought  it  useful,  at  first,  to  revert  to  the  degeneration  test, 
for  the  observations  mentioned  by  Garten^^  were  not  sufficient  in  num- 
ber, and  the  methods  for  electrical  detection  have  improved  since  that 
time.  30  animals  were  operated  on  and  examined  at  different  inter- 
vals after  nerve  sections  on  one  side.  Some  survived  more  than  2 
months,  and  this  was  sufficient  to  detect  histological  signs  of  alteration 
in  the  terminals  (Fessard  and  Pezard-°).  Such  signs  began  to  appear 
on  our  Torpedoes  only  5  or  6  weeks  after  the  operation  (average  tem- 
perature 14°  C).  Before  that,  the  organs  were  found  excitable,  al- 
though needing  more  and  more  current.  Excitability  in  any  form 
(electrical,  chemical,  mechanical)  totally  disappears  after  about  7 
weeks.  Osmic  acid  staining  then  shows  fragmentation  of  the  last 
branches.  Deprived  of  its  terminal  innervation,  the  electric  organ  is 
decidedly  incapable  of  activity. 

b.  No  sound  conclusion  can  be  deduced  from  the  old  results  on 
poisoning  by  curare.     Most  of  the  previous  experimenters   (namely^ 


506  ANNALS  NEW  YORK  ACADEMY  OF  SCIENCES 

Moreau,  Babuchin,  Gotch)  denied  its  action,  and  this  was,  at  first,  also 
our  opinion,  as  24  hour  immersions  in  1  per  cent  curare  solutions  had 
no  marked  effect.  In  other  experiments,  we  had  noted  that  hght  cuts 
made  with  a  razor  blade  along  the  longitudinal  surface  of  a  column  did 
not  interfere  with  its  capacity  for  delivering  good  responses.  We 
thought  that  this  treatment  might  facilitate  the  penetration  of  drugs, 
and  we  immediately  got  a  positive  result  (Auger  and  Fessard^).  1  per 
cent  solutions  acting  during  1-2  hours  abolish  all  excitability.  As  op- 
posed to  the  striated  muscle,  the  electric  organ  becomes  inexcitable  after 
curarization. 

c.  If  the  electric  plate  is  a  real  functional  unit,  its  activity  must  be 
greatly  impaired  or  completely  suppressed  by  severe  mutilation.  Iso- 
lated columns  were  divided  lengthwise  into  three  narrow  strips,  each 
plate  being  thus  fragmented  into  3  parts,  and  severely  damaged.  This 
is  a  complementary  situation,  compared  to  that  of  the  nerve  degenera- 
tion experiments,  the  plate  itself  being  practically  destroyed,  but  the 
finer  nerve  tracts  at  the  endings  being  only  partially  damaged.  In 
spite  of  this  drastic  treatment,  the  preparation  remains  excitable  and 
gives  discharges  of  smaller  amplitudes,  but  of  normal  shape. 

d.  One  may  object  to  the  strict  vahdity  of  arguments  (a)  and  (6), 
as  they  concern  situations  in  which  the  plate  is  modified  in  some  way. 
For  instance,  after  nerve  degeneration,  the  organ  shows  some  reduc- 
tion in  thickness,  and  the  curare  poisoning  may  have  altered  the  prop- 
erties of  the  plate.  Results  obtained  with  isolated  intact  columns  may 
supply  indirect,  but  more  satisfactory,  evidence. 

Using  very  strong  electrical  stimuli,  we  had  expected  to  get  a  true 
effector  response,  as  the  nerve  impulses  would  arrive  during  the  re- 
fractory period  of  this  hypothetical  effector  unit.  Different  electrode 
positions  were  tried,  the  results  of  which  we  observed  from  the  point 
of  view  of  threshold,  latency,  amplitude,  and  components  of  complex 
waves.  The  results  show  that  any  of  these  parameters  (and  the  varia- 
tions thereof)  depends  upon,  and  can  only  be  explained  by,  the  char- 
acteristics of  the  nerve  supply  pattern.  They  appear  to  be  determined 
by  the  symmetry  of  the  nerve  distribution,  not  by  that  of  the  plates. 
These  are  some  of  our  observations: 

1.  No  difference  in  latency  or  in  the  form  of  the  discharge  can  be 
observed  in  supra-maximal  longitudinal  excitation,  whether  the  cur- 
rent is  or  is  not  in  the  direction  of  the  discharge.  No  systematic  dif- 
ferences in  threshold  values  were  found. 


FESSARD:  ACTIVITY  OF  ELECTRIC  PLATES  507 

2.  In  the  lengthwise  stimulation  of  Torpedo  columns,  the  latency 
can  never  be  reduced  to  less  than  a  certain  minimum  (3  to  5  msec), 
however  strong  the  current  (even  if  we  approach  the  lethal  value). 
This  can  be  explained  by  the  special  distribution  of  nerves  in  Torpedo 
columns,  if  one  admits  that  excitation  is  always  localized  at  the  bend- 
ing points  of  the  nerve  branches.  The  situation  is  comparable  to  that 
in  which  Rushton^*  made  his  observations  on  excitation  of  bent  nerves. 
Before  entering  the  plates,  the  nerve  branches  run  along  the  edges  of 
the  prismatic  column,  then  each  axon  bends  at  right  angles  and,  by  mul- 
tiple division  at  the  bending  point,  sends  small  transverse  twigs  to  sev- 
eral plates.  This  point  {"bouquet  de  Wagner")  is  at  the  same  time  a 
Ranvier-node,  and  we  assume,  as  most  likely,  that  it  is  the  most  distal 
one  from  which  excitation  can  start  in  longitudinal  stimulation. 

3.  This  assumption  is  confirmed  by  the  fact  that  the  latency  is  not 
irreducible.  Strong  transverse  stimulations  lower  it  to  less  than  1 
msec.  (Auger  and  Fessard^),  clearly  showing  that  the  long  latencies 
are  due  to  nerve  conduction  in  the  plate  plane,  and  not  to  some  elabo- 
ration process  in  the  plate  itself.  When  the  intensity  is  lowered,  the 
transverse  latency  increases  regularly,  but  never  exceeds,  in  normal 
conditions,  the  shortest  latency  observed  in  longitudinal  stimulation. 
This  is  perfectly  comprehensible,  if  we  localize  the  excitation  at  bend- 
ing points  nearer  and  nearer  the  "bouquets  de  Wagner,"  provided  that 
we  adopt  the  current  opinion,  according  to  which  the  threshold  values 
diminish,  the  further  we  are  from  the  nerve  extremities. 

e.  The  non-existence  of  a  relay  action  similar  to  that  of  the  neuro- 
muscular command  is  further  indicated  by  the  absence  of  repetitive  re- 
sponse when  acetylcholine  is  injected  intra-arterially  into  an  isolated 
organ,^®'  ^^  although  we  have  shown  that  this  drug  exerts,  in  this  case, 
a  marked  depolarizing  effect. 

/.  Other  drugs  were  introduced  into  the  interior  of  the  plates  by  the 
same  technique  as  described  in  (6)  (curare  poisoning):  eserine(10"*), 
which  lengthens  up  to  more  than  4  times  the  declining  phase  of  the  ele- 
mentary discharge;  atropin  (lO"'^),  which  suppresses  all  excitability  in 
1-2  hours;  curare,  which  has  the  same  effect  as  atropin,  but  in  doses 
ten  times  larger.  During  the  course  of  both  intoxications,  the  threshold 
intensity  progressively  rises.  The  duration  of  the  elementary  dis- 
charge does  not  change  or  even  become  shorter.  These  data  confirm 
the  cholinergic  nature  of  the  nerves  supplying  the  organ.  They  also 
add  supplementary  evidence  in  favor  of  the  similitude  between  the 
electric  discharge  and  the  end-plate  potential  (cf.  Kuffler^*''  "). 


508  ANNALS  NEW  YORK  ACADEMY  OF  SCIENCES 

II.  ORIGIN  AND  LOCALIZATION  OF  THE  ELECTROMOTIVE 

FORCE 

These  are  largely  a  matter  of  speculation,  as  they  now  concern  the 
level  of  molecular  organization.  Thermodynamic  data  on  one  hand, 
experiments  with  microelectrodes  on  the  other,  would  be  most  useful. 
Awaiting  these,  we  must  content  ourselves  with  discussing  the  points 
on  v/hich  experimental  results  are  available:  for  instance,  the  most  im- 
portant problem  of  plate  polarization  in  the  resting  state. 

a.  The  classical  hypothesis  is  that  of  Bernstein.''  This  postulates  a 
permanent  superficial  polarization  of  the  plate  boundaries.  The  arrival 
of  a  nerve  impulse  results  in  a  local  transient  annulment  of  this  polari- 
zation, in  accordance  with  the  general  assumption.  Now,  only  one 
side  of  each  plate  is  innervated  and  capable  of  being  depolarized.  As, 
at  rest,  the  potential  difference  between  the  two  extremities  of  an  elec- 
tric organ  is  approximately  zero,  in  spite  of  the  coupling  in  series  of 
the  plates,  one  must  suppose  an  exact  compensation  of  the  electromo- 
tive ft)rce  developed  on  one  side  of  each  plate  by  that  developed  on  the 
other  side  (figure  2,  Schema  I).  We  thus  arrive  at  that  strange  con- 
ception of  two  distant  polarized  layers,  endowed  with  different  prop- 
el ties  and  yet  electrically  charged  in  exactly  the  same  way.  Their 
properties  are  different,  because  one  is  supposed  to  discharge  itself 
through  a  sudden  internal  leak  due  to  collapse,  while  the  other  starts 
discharging  without  collapse  into  the  external  medium.  Yet  not  a 
sign  of  a  double  evolution  of  potential  can  be  observed  in  the  course 
of  the  elementary  discharge.  On  the  other  hand,  these  opposite  layers 
are  not  situated  in  similar  regions,  from  the  point  of  view  of  tissue 
structure  and  chemical  environment.  It  is  very  unlikely  that  they 
should  develop  the  same  electromotive  force. 

b.  Another  hypothesis  has  been  recently  proposed  by  Cox,  Coates, 
and  Vertncr  Brown,' '^  who  assume  a  constantly  present  electromotive 
force,  non-compens.ited  by  another  opposed  electromotive  force,  but 
hidden  by  tlie  high  resistivity  of  an  interface.  This  is  not  conceivable, 
in  our  opinion,  without  caj^acitive  properties  by  which  the  interface 
appears  as  passively  charged  (fijure  2,  Schema  ITl.  According  to  the 
present  concept,  "the  discharge  would  be  started  by  a  very  large  and 
rapid  drop  in  the  resistance."  We  have  seen  that  this  drop  in  resist- 
ance really  exists,  but  we  cannot  conceive  of  the  resting  voltage,  sup- 
posedly present,  being  lowered,  say  from  500  volts  to  5  millivolts,  by  the 
simple  interposition  of  biological  membranes,  the  resistances  of  which 
are,  at  the  highest  estimate,  1000  ohms/cm.^  in  the  nerve  interfaces 


FESSARD:  ACTIVITY  OF  ELECTRIC  PLATES  509 

(Cole  and  Hodgkin^^).  Furthermore,  electrometric  determinations  on 
isolated  portions  freed  from  the  internal  derivations  normally  present 
in  the  intact  animal,  should  reveal  much  higher  resting  potentials.  This 
was  never  observed. 

c.  It  appears  to  us  that  the  following  question  should  be  resolved 
first:  Do  polarized  layers  really  exist  in  the  plate,  previous  to  its  state 
of  activity?  To  prove  this,  it  is  necessary  to  communicate  in  some 
way  with  the  interior  of  the  plates. 

1.  We  have  tried  piercing  slowly  a  column  from  the  electric  organ 
of  Torpedo  with  a  fine  metallic  electrode  and  have  observed  small  re- 
petitive discharges,  due  to  a  mechanical  excitation,  which  we  have  shown 
to  be  caused  by  irritation  of  the  nerve  twigs  encountered.^  When  the 
electrode  is  extremely  fine,  a  number  of  plates  may  be  perforated 
without  being  excited  and  without  giving  rise  to  those  systematic  vari- 
ations in  potential  we  might  expect  when  passing  through  one  plate  to 
the  following. 

2.  We  also  took  small  groups  of  columns,  one  of  which  we  slit  lat- 
erally with  the  edge  of  a  heated  blade.  The  measurements  were  started 
immediately  with  the  ordinary  method  of  opposition,  one  electrode  be- 
ing placed  on  the  killed  region,  the  other  as  far  as  possible  from  it,  on 
the  intact  tissue  of  the  same  column.  The  resting  potential  had  to  be 
dissociated  from  the  long-lasting  residuals  of  activity,  following  the 
excitation  produced  by  the  lesion  (cf.  Gotch^-).  Three  methods  were 
tried:  first,  allowing  the  residuals  to  vanish;  second,  diminishing  their 
disturbing  effect  by  a  transverse  arrangement  of  the  electrodes;  third, 
using  a  degenerated  preparation. 

The  results  in  these  3  cases  are  exemplified  in  figure  1.  In  figure 
1  (2),  the  A  electrode,  being  a  little  more  dorsal  than  B,  is  positive 
at  the  start,  according  to  the  direction  of  the  discharges.  However,  it 
rapidly  reverts  to  its  steady  potential  value,  which  is  negative,  rela- 
tively, to  B.     In  (3),  no  initial  discharge  is  present  as  expected. 

In  all  cases,  no  value  higher  than  5  mv.  has  been  obtained  for  this 
rest  potential.  This  is  20  to  30  times  less  than  the  elementary  dis- 
charge. We  cannot  believe  that  such  a  discrepancy  can  be  completely 
due  to  a  shunt  effect. 

The  preceding  results,  incomplete  as  they  are,  throw  a  serious  doubt 
on  the  value  of  the  first  and  second  hypotheses.  The  alleged  perma- 
nent polarization  may  not,  therefore,  exist,  at  least  not  at  sufficient 
strength  to  play  the  more  important  part  in  the  discharge.  This  sug- 
gests a  third  hypothesis  that  we  formulated  once,*  and  according  to 


510 


ANNALS  NEW  YORK  ACADEMY  OF  SCIENCES 


B 


m 


i 


i 


ded. 

o 


I 

I 

.4. 


•minutes 


FiGURB    1.    Measurements   of   potential    differences    (Va-Vb)    immediately    following   a   localized 
injury  (hatched  zone). 

Torpedo  organ,  3  different  cases  (see  text). 

which  the  activity  in  the  electric  organ  simply  consists  in  the  transient 
appearance  of  a  membrane  polarization,  rather  than  in  the  depolariza- 
tion of  a  previously  polarized  surface.  This  is  the  meaning  of  the 
schema  represented  in  figure  2,  Schema  III.     However,  it  is  difficult 


M 

+ 

— 

— 

+ 

4- 

— 

1 

— 

+ 

+ 

— 

— 

+ 

+ 

— 

+ 

Figure  2.  Hypothetical  schemas  proposed  to  explain  the  production  of  an  electromotive  force 
in  electric  organs. 

N  indicates  the  innervated  side  of  the  plate.  The  dotted  lines  represent  what  is  supposed  to 
collapse  during  activity'. 

The  dotted  lines  in  II  are  intended  to  show  a  passively  charged  membrane  in  contrast  with 
other  interfaces,  which  are  supposed  to  be  actively  polarized  by  an  internal  electromotive  force. 

The  figure  makes  it  clear  that  IV  may  be  described  as  a  synthesis  of  I,  II,  and  III. 

to  believe  in  the  formation  of  a  polarized  layer  at  the  moment  when  the 
responsible  membrane  collapses,  as  is  indicated  by  the  drop  of  its  re- 
sistance. This  schema,  like  the  other  two,  appears  really  far  too 
simple. 


FESSARD:  ACTIVITY  OF  ELECTRIC  PLATES  511 

Finally,  the  schema  that  we  shall  tentatively  propose  as  the  most 
representative  of  our  present  knowledge  and  the  most  promising  as 
help  for  future  research,  is  the  one  shown  in  figure  2,  Schema  IV.  It  is 
supposed  to  represent  a  complex  molecular  structure  of  the  same  type 
as  that  used  by  modern  biophysicists  in  their  hypotheses  on  the  consti- 
tution of  the  molecular  membranes  (Danielli  and  Davson^^.  Such 
double  layer  leaflets  are  built  up  with  lipoid  and  protein  molecules,  and 
are  widespread  at  the  cell  surfaces.  Similar  arrangements  have  been 
assumed  to  be  present,  for  instance,  in  the  rods  of  the  retina,  which  has 
well-known  electrical  properties. 

Figure  2  makes  it  clear  how  this  last  hypothetical  schema  may  be 
considered  as  a  synthesis  of  the  other  three.  In  effect,  it  borrows  an 
idea  from  each  of  the  preceding  theories.  It  is  like  the  first,  inasmuch 
as  it  admits  the  presence  of  two  opposing  polarized  layers,  of  which 
only  one  can  be  neutralized.  It  borrows  from  the  second  the  idea  that 
a  high  resistivity  layer  exists  (probably  made  of  oriented  lipoid  chains 
in  the  intermediate  region),  which  collapses  during  the  discharge;  and 
it  is  in  agreement  with  the  third,  in  admitting  that  the  electromotive 
force  is  strictly  localized  at  the  innervated  face  of  the  plate,  where  it 
becomes  apparent  during  the  short  period  when  one  of  the  layers  is 
depolarized. 

BIBLIOGRAPHY 

1.  Auger,  D.,  &  A.  Fessaxd 

1938.  C.  R.  Soc.  Biol.  128:  1067. 

2.  Auger,  D.,  &  A.  Fessard 

1939.  Ibid.  131:765. 

3.  Auger,  D.,  &  A.  Fessard 
1939.     Ann.  de  Physiol.  15:  261. 

4.  Auger,  D.,  &  A.  Fessard 

1939.     Livro  de  Homenagem  Prof.  A.  e  M.  Ozorio  de  Almeida.  25.     Rio  de  Ja- 
neiro, Brazil. 

6.  Auger,  D.,  &  A.  Fessard 
1941.     C.  R.  Soc.  Biol.  135:  76. 

6.  Auger,  D.,  &  A.  Fessard 
1929.    Ibid.  102:582. 

7.  Bailey,  K. 

1939.     Biochem.  J.  33 :  255. 

8.  Baldwin,  E.,  &  D.  Needham 

1937.     Proc.  Roy.  Soc.  London  B  122:  197. 

9.  Bernstein,  J. 

1912.     Elektrobiologie.  118. 

10.  Bernstein,  J.,  &  A.  Tschermak 
1906.     Pflueger's  Arch.  112:  439. 

11.  Coates,  C,  &  R.  Cox 

1945.     Zoologica30:89. 


512  ANNALS  NEW  YORK  ACADEMY  OF  SCIENCES 

12.  Cole,  K.,  &  H.  Curtis 

1939.     J.  Cien.  Physiol.  22:  649. 

13.  Cole,  K.,  &  A.  Hod?kin 
1939.     .J.  Gen.  Physiol.  22:  671. 

14.  Couteaux,  R. 

194.5.     C.  R.  Soc.  Biol.  139:641. 

15.  Cox,  R.,  C.  Coates,  &  V.  Brown 
1945.     J.  Gen.  Physiol.  28:  187. 

16.  Curtis,  H.,  &  K.  Cole 

1942.  .J.  Gell.  &  Comp.  Physiol.  19:  135. 

17.  Danielli,  J.,  &  H.  Davson 

1943.  The  Permeability  of  Natural  Membranes.     Cambridge. 

18.  Feldberg,  W.,  A.  Fessard,  &  D.  Nachmansohn 

1939.  J.  Physiol.  97:2. 

19.  Feldberg,  W.,  &  A.  Fe3sard 
1942.     J.  Physiol.  101:200. 

20.  Fessard,  A.,  &  A.  Pezard 

1940.  C.  R.  Soc.  Biol.  134:  525. 

21.  Garten,  S. 

1910.     Handb.  d.  vergl.  Physiol.  105. 

22.  Gotch,  F. 

1887.  Phil.  Trans.  B.  487. 

23.  Gotch,  F. 

1888.  Phil.  Trans.  B.  329. 

24.  Katz,  B. 

1942.     J.  Neurophysiol.  5:  169 

25.  Kisch,  B. 

1930.     Biochem.  Z.  225:  183. 

26.  Kuffler,  S. 

1942.  J.  Neurophysiol.  5:  18. 

27.  Kuffler,  S. 

1943.  Ibid.  6:99. 

28.  Marey,  E. 

1887.     Travau.x  du  Laboratoire  de  1'  Institut  Marey  3:  1. 

29.  Meyaihof,  O. 

1926.  Thermodynamik  des  Lebensprozesses.     Handb.  d.  Physik  11:  254. 

30.  Nachmansohn,  D. 

1945.     Vitamins  and  Hormones  3:  337. 

31.  Nachmansohn,  D.,  R.  Cox,  C.  Coates,  &  A.  Machado 

1942.  J.  Neurophysiol.  5:  499. 

32.  Nachmansohn,  D.,  R.  Cox,  C.  Coates,  &  A.  Machado 

1943.  Ibid.  6:383. 

33.  Rosenberg,  H. 

1918.     Handb.  der  norm.  u.  pathol.  Physiol.  8:  876. 

34.  Rushton,  W. 

1927.  J.  Physiol.  63:3.57. 


FESSARD:  ACTIVITY  OF  ELECTRIC  PLATES  513 


PLATE  4 


514  ANNALS  NEW  YORK  ACADEMY  OF  SCIENCES 


Plate  4 

A.  Middle  record:  response  of  an  isolated  column  (Torpedo  organ)  to  an  elec- 
trical stimulus  directed  along  its  main  axis. 

Ivower :  response  of  a  small  fragment  of  tissue ;  transverse  stimulation ;  thresh- 
old intensity. 

Upper:  single  fiber  action  potential  (electric  organ  nerve). 
Time  scales  in  milliseconds. 

B.  Double  beam  oscillograph  record. 

Above:  discharge  of  a  fragment  of  electric  tissue  (Raia). 

Below:  impedance  change  test  with  an  alternating  ciuTent  of  15,000  cycles. 

The  whole  activity  wave  lasts  12  milliseconds. 


Annals  N.  Y.  Acad.  Scr. 


Vol..  XI.VII,  Aht.  4,  PuTK  i 


FKSSARl):   ACTIVITY   OK   Kl.KCTRIC   PI.ATK;- 


PHYSIOLOGICAL  FUNCTION  FROM  THE 
STANDPOINT  OF  ENZYME  CHEMISTRY 

By  D.  E.  Green 

Departments  of  Medicine  and  Biochemistry,  College  of  Physicians  and  Surgeons, 
Coluinbia  University,  New  York,  N.  Y. 

It  is  a  curious  fact  tliat,  although  there  is  general  recognition  and 
agreement  that  the  cell  is  a  chemical  system,  none  the  less  the  full  im- 
plications of  this  truism  have  yet  to  be  appreciated  in  some  fields  of 
physiological  investigation.  Perhaps  the  explanation  is  to  be  found  in 
the  preoccupation  of  biochemistry,  until  very  recently,  with  problems 
of  the  structure  of  cellular  constituents  and  with  their  estimation. 
Classical  biochemistry  represented  to  the  physiologist  the  extension  of 
histology  to  the  chemical  field.  The  study  of  what  we  may  call  chem- 
ical morphology  was  hardly  calculated  to  attract  physiologists  or  to 
arouse  their  interest  in  the  chemical  basis  of  physiological  function. 
However,  the  interest  of  biochemistry  has  been  shifting  gradually  from 
the  purely  structural  problems  to  the  dynamic  chemical  events  of  the 
cell.  Our  present  knowledge  of  the  chemical  mechanisms  of  the  cell 
has  grown  sufficiently  for  it  to  be  ignored  no  longer  by  those  who  are 
concerned  with  the  study  of  physiological  function. 

We  may  conceive  of  the  cell  as  a  chemical  factory  in  which  literally 
thousands  of  chemical  reactions  take  place,  cheek  by  jowl,  without 
mutual  interference.  Some  of  these  reactions  are  concerned  in  the  syn- 
thesis of  structural  components  of  the  cell  and  others  in  providing 
chemical  energy  for  carrying  on  the  activities  of  the  cell.  Practically 
without  exception,  these  reactions  do  not  proceed  spontaneously.  They 
require  the  presence  of  protein  catalysts,  which  we  call  enzymes.  Each 
enzyme  is  distinct,  chemically,  from  all  the  others,  and  is  uniquely 
specialized  for  its  particular  catalysis.  If  this  picture  of  the  cell  is 
correct,  then  it  follows  that  all  dynamic  activities  including  physiolog- 
ical function  must  be  reducible  to  terms  of  enzyme  chemistry.  In  other 
words,  physiological  function  and  enzyme  chemistry  are  two  sides  of 
the  same  coin.  I  hope,  in  the  short  time  at  my  disposal,  to  marshal 
the  available  evidence  which  justifies  this  interpretation. 

In  the  syndromes  of  avitaminosis,  we  observe  profound  morphological 
and  physiological  abnormalities.  The  recognizable  signs  of  each  of  the 
vitamin  deficiencies  are  too  well  known  to  be  discussed  here.    The  point 

(515) 


516  ANNALS  NEW  YORK  ACADEMY  OF  SCIENCES 

I  want  to  make,  however,  is  that  these  abnormalities  which  cover  the  en- 
tire gamut  of  physiological  dysfunction  can  be  explained  completely  in 
terms  of  certain  enzyme  systems.  Vitamin  Bi,  B2,  Bg,  and  the  P-P 
factor  have  all  been  shown  to  be  the  prosthetic  groups  of  certain 
enzyme  systems.  When  these  vitamins  are  not  available  in  the  diet, 
the  active  enzymes  cannot  be  formed  in  the  cell.  In  consequence  of 
the  failure  of  these  enzyme  systems  to  function  properly,  an  abnormal 
physiological  situation  develops,  which,  if  uncorrected,  will  lead  to 
death.  Which  organ  first  registers  the  effect  of  a  particular  deficiency 
is  determined  by  the  amount  of  the  reserves  of  enzymes  containing  the 
vitamin  and  by  the  relative  importance  of  this  set  of  enzymes  in  the 
economy  of  the  organ.  Thus,  in  Bi  deficiency  in  the  pigeon,  the  brain 
is  the  first  organ  to  register  disturbed  function,  presumably  because 
there  are  no  reserves  of  this  vitamin  in  the  brain  and  because  the 
active  enzyme  formed  by  the  vitamin  plays  a  key  role  in  the  metab- 
olism of  brain.  One  may  well  raise  the  point  that,  if,  as  in  the  avita- 
minosis,  the  causal  link  between  the  physiological  disturbance  and  the 
effect  on  enzyme  systems  is  unquestioned,  then  surely  there  is  a  good 
case  for  assuming  the  same  link  between  the  normal  physiology  and 
enzyme  systems. 

Woolley^  has  introduced  the  use  of  anti-vitamin  reagents  which,  by 
virtue  of  their  structural  resemblance  to  the  vitamins,  are  able  to  pre- 
vent the  vitamins  either  from  being  incorporated  into  enzymes  or 
from  functioning  as  prosthetic  groups.  These  anti-vitamins  produce, 
in  a  relatively  short  period,  the  same  syndromes  which  arise  from  de- 
priving the  diet  of  an  animal  of  a  particular  vitamin.  Not  only  are 
these  anti-vitamins  valuable  for  speeding  up  the  process  of  avita- 
minosis,  but  the  profound  pharmacological  effects  which  they  induce 
permit  correlations  between  the  action  of  reagents  on  certain  enzyme 
systems  and  the  pharmacological  consequences.  In  other  words,  the 
anti-vitamins  have  focussed  attention  on  the  fact  that  the  effects  of 
certain,  if  not  all,  pharmacological  agents  can  be  explained  completely 
in  terms  of  effects  on  enzyme  systems.^'  ^  In  recent  years,  a  rich  litera- 
ture has  grown  up  to  deal  with  this  correlation.  There  are  now  at  least 
14  instances  in  which  the  pharmacological  effects  of  certain  reagents  can 
be  explained  in  terms  of  a  specific  effect  on  an  enzyme  system.  Thus, 
iodoacetic  acid,  which  induces  muscle  rigor,  has  been  found  to  inhibit, 
in  minute  concentration,  the  triosephosphoric  dehydrogenase  which 
catalyzes  an  essential  step  in  lactic  acid  formation.  This  paralysis  of 
the  triosephosphoric  dehydrogenase  accounts  for  all  the  pharmacolog- 
ical effects  produced  by  iodoacetic  acid.     Fluoroacetic  acid,  the  highly 


GREEN :  PHYtilOlJJGICAL  FUNCTION,  ENZ YME  C HEM  1ST R  Y      5^7 

toxic  agent  discovered  in  Chemical  Warfare  Research,  has  been  shown 
to  inhibit  the  enzyme  systems  involved  in  the  metaboHsm  of  acetic 
acid.  The  classical  pharmacological  reagents,  strychnine,  eserine,  and 
prostigmine,  have  been  shown  to  exert  their  effects  exclusively  by  virtue 
of  their  paralysis  of  cholinesterase.  The  effects  of  cyanide  on  cyto- 
chrome oxidase,  fluoride  on  enolase,  and  chlorine  on  the  triosephos- 
phoric  dehydrogenase,  are  other  examples  of  this  correlation.  Perhaps 
even  more  unexpected,  has  been  the  identification  of  various  toxins  with 
enzymes.  Thus,  spreading  factor,  the  agent  which  facilitates  the 
rapid  diffusion  of  injected  substances  through  dermal  tissue,  has  been 
shown  to  be  identical  with  hyaluronidase,  a  mucolytic  splitting  enzyme. 
The  hemolytic  principle  of  Clostridium  welchii  toxin  and  that  of  snake 
venom  have  been  shown  to  be  lecithinases,  and  the  hemolytic  effects 
are  completely  explicable  in  terms  of  their  ability  to  weaken  the  lipoid 
membrane  of  the  red  blood  cell  by  hydrolysis  of  the  lecithin  contained 
therein.  During  the  war,  some  English  workers,  led  by  McFarlane,* 
identified  one  of  the  toxins  produced  by  the  gas  gangrene  organism  as 
collagenase,  a  proteolytic  enzyme  which  dissolves  the  connective  tissue 
sheath  of  muscle.  The  action  of  this  enzyme  explains  the  pulping  of 
muscle  observable  in  advanced  cases  of  gas  gangrene  poisoning.  At 
the  present  moment,  it  would  be  premature  to  assume  that  all  specific 
pharmacological  agents  which  work  at  high  dilutions  are  active  by 
virtue  of  their  effects  on  enzyme  systems.  On  the  other  hand,  it  is 
pertinent  to  point  out  that  no  other  principle  of  mechanism  has  been 
established  for  any  pharmacological  agent  which  has  been  studied. 
Apart  from  the  dictates  of  caution,  there  is  no  good  reason  not  to 
anticipate  that,  eventually,  all  effects  of  specific  pharmacological  agents 
will  be  reducible  to  terms  of  enzyme  chemistry. 

The  study  of  endocrines  has  always  been  one  of  the  most  active  fields 
of  physiological  investigation,  and  it  is  of  interest  to  inquire  to  what 
extent  hormones  can  be  related  to  enzyme  phenomena.  Until  quite 
recently,  hormones  were  held  up  as  notable  exceptions  to  the  rule  that 
substances  which  act  at  high  dilutions  must  be  enzymes  or  parts  of 
enzymes,  or  must  specifically  affect  some  enzyme  system.  Some  recent 
research,  however,  fails  to  confirm  the  hormones  as  exceptions  to  the 
enzyme-trace  substance  thesis.  No  doubt,  everyone  is  aware  of  the 
epoch-making  discovery  of  Cori  and  his  group,^  that  one  of  the  hor- 
mones of  the  anterior  pituitary  inhibits  the  action  of  hexokinase,  and 
that,  in  turn,  this  inhibition  is  released  by  insulin.  We  have  here 
a  clear  blueprint  for  the  way  in  which  hormone  antagonism  can  be 
effected.     A  key  enzyme  system  which  controls  some  metabolic  process 


518  ANNALS  NEW  YORK  ACADEMY  OF  SCIENCES 

can  be  regulated  by  a  set  of  hormones,  one  of  which  inhibits,  while  the 
other  releases  the  inhibition.  All  students  of  endocrinology  have  long 
been  aware  that  hormones  regulate  metabolic  processes,  and  it  is  not 
surprising  to  find  in  one  instance,  at  any  rate,  that  the  regulation  oper- 
ates at  the  level  of  the  enzyme  systems.  Houssay  and  his  colleagues 
in  the  Argentine  have  presented  cogent  evidence  that  renin,  a  kidney 
hormone,  is  a  type  of  proteolytic  enzyme  which  hydrolyzes  one  of  the 
plasma  proteins  to  form  a  pressor  substance.  In  this  instance,  the 
hormone  regulates  metabolic  processes  by  actually  assuming  an  enzy- 
matic role. 

In  still  another  direction,  there  has  been  confirmation  that  enzymic 
phenomena  underlie  essential  physiological  processes.  The  brilliant 
work  of  Beadle*'  and  his  school  have  made  it  abundantly  clear  that  the 
regulation  of  growth  and  development  by  the  hereditary  units  of  the 
cell,  viz.,  the  genes,  is  exercised  through  control  of  enzyme  systems. 
They  have  shown  that  each  gene  determines  the  synthesis,  probably,  of 
a  single  enzyme.  Whereas  some  of  the  hormones  regulate  metabolic 
reactions  by  slowing  up  or  speeding  up  an  enzyme  reaction,  genes 
regulate  by  determining  the  synthesis  of  an  enzyme.  Remarkably  little 
is  known  of  the  mechanisms  by  which  enzymes  are  synthesized,  but  it 
would  appear  that,  whatever  the  mechanism,  the  genie  material  will  be 
implicated. 

The  mere  recognition  that  enzymic  phenomena  underlie  physiological 
function  is,  of  course,  only  the  first  step  in  the  biochemical  analysis. 
Obviously,  the  exercise  of  physiological  function  requires  a  source  of 
energy,  and  the  energy  must  arise  in  enzyme-catalyzed  reactions.  But 
how  is  the  energy  converted  into  the  manifold  forms  required  by  the 
cell?  How  is  chemical  energy  converted  into  mechanical  energy  of 
contraction  or  electrical  energy  of  nervous  conduction  (to  mention 
two  examples)  ?  There  are  no  transforming  elements  in  the  cell,  such 
as  the  storage  battery.  Until  recently,  this  problem  of  energy  con- 
versions was  shrouded  in  deepest  fog,  but  some  light  has  managed  to 
penetrate.  It  now  appears,  from  the  work  of  various  laboratories,  that 
the  contraction  of  the  myosin  molecule  may  be  coupled  with  the 
enzymatic  hydrolysis  of  adenosine  triphosphate.  The  contraction  of 
muscle  is  now  visualized  as  the  integration  of  the  single  contractions 
of  myosin  molecules  arranged  in  linear  series.  The  picture  is,  of 
course,  very  crude,  and  probably  will  be  modified  by  further  research. 
However,  if  the  basic  facts  are  correct,  then  we  have  a  blueprint  for 
visualizing  energy  transfers  at  the  enzyme  level.  Adenosine  triphos- 
phate represents  a  readily  tapped  supply  of  the  chemical  energy  gen- 


GREEN:  PHYSIOLOGICAL  FUNCTION,  ENZYME  CHEMISTRY      5^9 

erated  by  the  process  of  glycolysis.  Because  of  the  close  proximity 
of  adenosine  triphosphatase  and  myosin,  some  of  the  chemical  energy 
of  hydrolysis  is  absorbed  by  the  myosin  molecule,  which  then  under- 
goes simultaneous  contraction.  In  other  words,  myosin  is  acting  as  a 
kind  of  transformer  element  for  the  conversion  of  chemical  energy  to 
mechanical  energy. 

We  have  to  consider  the  possibility  that,  just  as  myosin  is  specialized 
for  muscular  contraction,  chlorophyll  or  visual  purple  for  photochem- 
ical reactions,  and  hemoglobin  for  oxygen  transfer,  so  there  may  be  one 
or  more  proteins  in  nerve  specialized  for  the  reactions  which  underlie 
the  propagation  of  a  nerve  impulse.  The  knowledge  that  acetylcholine 
and  adrenaline  are  the  chemical  agents  involved  in  nerve  conduction, 
is  merely  the  introduction  to  the  problem  of  mechanism.  Undoubtedly, 
these  substances  react  with  special  proteins.  It  is  the  transformations 
which  these  special  proteins  then  undergo  that  is  the  basis  of  the  phe- 
nomenon of  nerve  transmission. 

REFERENCES 

1.  Woolley,  D.  W. 

1945.  Science.  100:  579. 

2.  Green,  D.  E. 

1946.  Currents  in  Biochemical  Research:  149.     Interscience  Publishers,  Inc., 
New  York. 

3.  Green,  D.  E. 

1941.     Advances  in  Enzvmology.  1:  177.     Interscience  Publishers,  Inc.,   New 
York. 

4.  McFarlane,  R.  G.,  &  J.  D.  MacLennan 
1945.     Lancet  2:  328. 

5.  Price,  W.  H.,  Carl  F.  Cori,  &  Sidney  P.  Colowick 

1945.  J.  Biol.  Chem.  160:  633. 

6.  Beadle,  G.  W. 

1946.  Currents  in  Biochemical  Research:  1.     Interscience  Publishers,    Inc., 
New  York. 


CHOLINESTERASE 

By  Oscar  Bodansky* 

Medical  Division,  Chemical  Warfare  Service,  Edgewood  Arsenal,  Maryland^ 

On  the  basis  of  his  studies  on  the  pharmacology  of  acetylcholine,  in 
1914,  Dale  stated:  "In  the  blood  at  body  temperatures  it  seems  not  im- 
probable that  an  esterase  contributes  to  the  removal  of  the  active 
ester  from  circulation."^  In  1926,  Loewi  and  Navratil  observed  that 
acetylcholine,  as  well  as  "vagus  substance,"  was  rendered  inactive  by  in- 
cubation with  heart  extract.^  However,  such  inactivation  did  not 
occur  after  the  heart  extract  had  been  heated  or  subjected  to  ultra- 
violet irradiation.  These  observations  by  Loewi  and  Navratil  in- 
augurated the  study  of  the  enzyme,  cholinesterase. 

A  heat-labile  substance  which  is  capable  of  hydrolyzing  acetylcholine 
is  found  very  widely  distributed  in  the  organs  and  fluids  of  the  body. 
It  cannot  be  assumed  that,  apart  from  this  common  property  of  hy- 
drolyzing acetylcholine,  the  other  properties  of  this  enzyme  are  the 
same  in  all  these  tissues.  Our  present  discussion  should  most  fit- 
tingly concern  itself  with  the  properties  of  this  enzyme  as  found  in 
nervous  tissue,  and  should  determine  the  extent  to  which  these  proper- 
ties play  a  role  in  nerve  activity.  However,  most  of  the  data  avail- 
able for  discussion  describe  chiefly  the  properties  of  cholinesterase 
found  in  serum,  red  cells,  and,  to  a  lesser  extent,  in  the  whole  brain. 
The  extent  to  which  these  data  apply  to  the  properties  of  cholinesterase, 
at  synapses  and  in  other  nerve  tissue,  should  be  carefully  evaluated. 

RELATION  BETWEEN  SUBSTRATE  CONCENTRATION  AND 

REACTION  VELOCITY 

We  shall  first  turn  our  attention  to  the  relation  between  the  rate  of 
action  of  cholinesterase  and  the  concentration  of  the  substrate,  acetyl- 
choline. Examination  of  the  data  shows  that  two  types  of  relation- 
ships hold.  The  first  type  appears  to  follow  the  Michaelis-Menten 
formulation:^ 

-^  =       ^  (1) 

where  v  is  the  reaction  velocity  at  substrate  concentration  S,  F^ax 
is  the  maximum  reaction  velocity  occurring  at  infinite  substrate  con- 


*  Lt.  Colonel,  M.  C,  A.  U.  S. 

t  Present  address:  Dept.  of  Pharmacology,  Cornell  University  Medical  College,  New  York,  N.  Y. 

(521) 


522  ANNALS  NEW  YORK  ACADEMY  OF  SCIENCES 

centration,  and  Kg  is  the  dissociation  constant  of  the  intermediate 
enzyme-substrate  complex. 

The  second  type  of  relation  obtaining  among  some  cholinesterases 
appears  to  be  one  in  which  inhibition  of  reaction  velocity  occurs  at 
higher  substrate  concentrations.  This  relation  has  been  found  to  hold 
for  some  other  enzymes,  such  as  lipases,  catalase,  oxygenase.*  The 
reactions  between  substrate  and  enzyme  may  be  formulated  as  follows: 
E  +  S  ?^  ES  (active) 

ES  +  (71  -  l)S:f±  ESn  (inactive) 

ES  ^E  +  P. 

The  relation  between  substrate  and  reaction  velocity  has  been  devel- 
oped by  Haldane^  for  the  reaction  where  n  =  2,  as  follows: 

'   max       *-' 


V    = 


S" 
S  +  A%  +  ^ 

A2 


(2) 


where  K2  is  the  dissociation  constant  of  the  inactive  enzyme-substrate 

r  p  on  r  oin— i 

compound  - — rlF^^ —    When  the  velocity  is  plotted  against  the  log- 

[Ebn\ 

arithm  of  the  substrate  concentration,  a  bell-shaped  curve  is  obtained. 
The  values  for  K,,  Ko  may  be  obtained  by  transformation  of  the 
above  equations,  according  to  the  method  of  Lineweaver  and  Burk.* 
Since  there  are  very  few  and  incomplete  data  in  the  literature  for 
cholinesterases  which  follow  this  type  of  substrate-reaction  velocity 
relationship,  we  shall  not  attempt  such  an  evaluation. 

In  TABLE  1,  are  listed  the  results  of  investigations  on  the  relationship 
between  concentration  of  the  substrate,  acetylcholine,  and  the  velocity 
of  cholinesterase  action.  It  may  be  seen  that  the  chohnesterases 
present  in  human  and  dog  serum,  and  in  the  cat  superior  cervical  gang- 
lion, show  increasing  rates  of  reaction,  with  increasing  substrate  con- 
centrations, which  become  asymptotic  to  a  maximal  rate  at  infinite 
substrate  concentration.  These  values  of  the  dissociation  constants 
(molar  concentration  at  which  one-half  maximal  reaction  velocity  oc- 
curs) range,  in  general,  from  1  to  1.7  X  10"^  In  contrast,  the  cholin- 
esterases from  the  red  cells  of  man,  sheep,  horse,  and  ox,  and  from  the 
brain  of  the  mouse  and  dog,  do  not  show  increasing  reaction  velocities 
with  increasing  substrate  concentration.  According  to  Mendel  and 
Rudney,"  and  to  Alles  and  Hawes,''  they  show  optimal  activity  at 
about  1  X  10-^M;  at  concentrations  higher  than  this,  the  reaction 
velocities  decrease. 

On  the  other  hand,  values  for  the  optimal  concentration  obtained  by 
Zeller  and  Bissegger,^^  and  by  Nachmansohn  and  Rothenberg,"  are 


BOD  AN  SKY:  CHOLINESTERASE 


523 


Table  1 

Relation  between  Reaction  Velocity  of  Cholinesterase  Action  and  Acetyl- 
choline Concentration 


In  accord  with  Michaelis-M enten  forvuda 


Enzyme  source 


Investigator 


Dissociation 
constant 


Dog  serum 
Dog  serum 
Dog  serum 
Human  serum 
Human  serum 
Cat  superior 

cervical  ganglion 


Goldstein^ 

Eadie^ 

Wright  &  Sabine* 

Click' 

Wright  &  Sabine* 

Click  i« 


1.25  X  10-3 
1.7  X  10-3 
0.26  X  10-3 

1.1  X  10-3 

1.2  X  10-3 

1       X  10-3 


Inhibition  at  higher  substrate  concentrations  (bell-shaped  curve) 


Enzyme  source 

Investigator 

Optimal 
concentration  (M) 

Red  cell:  human 
Red  cell:  human 

Red  cell:  human 
sheep 
horse 
ox 

Red  cell:  human 

Brain:  mouse 
dog 

Brain:  human 
Brain:  mouse 
Brain  areas:  ox 

AUes  &  Hawes" 
Zeller  &  Bissegger'^ 

Mendel  &  Rudney'3 

Nachmansohn  &  Rothenberg'^ 
Mendel  &  Rudney" 

Zeller  &  Bissegger^^ 
Nachmansohn  &  Rothenberg" 
Nachmansohn  &  Rothenberg'^ 

0.25  X  10-^ 
4.4    X  10-3 

<1.5    X  10-^ 

ca.     5       X  10-3 
<1.5    X  10^ 

4.1    X  10-3 
ca.     6       X  10-3 

ca.     8       X  10-3 

much  higher.  As  Mendel  and  Rudney^'^  have  pointed  out,  and  as  will 
be  seen  later,  the  concentration  of  electrolyte  influences  the  optimal 
concentration  of  substrate. 

The  relation  existing  between  reaction  velocity  and  substrate  con- 
centration of  acetylcholine  may  be  of  physiologic  significance.  Glick^° 
found  that,  under  optimal  substrate  conditions,  pH  7.4  and  38°,  0.10  y 
acetylcholine  chloride  was  split  per  second,  per  milligram  of  cat  supe- 
rior cervical  ganglion.  Brown  and  Feldberg^**  found  that  the  concen- 
tration of  acetylcholine  in  the  superior  cervical  ganglion  of  the  cat  was 


524  ANNALS  NEW  YORK  ACADEMY  OF  SCIENCES 

22.5y/gm.  of  tissue.  If  conditions  of  maximal  substrate  concentration 
are  assumed  to  exist,  0.225  seconds  would  be  required  to  split  the 
acetylcholine.  If  the  assumption  is  made,  that  lower  substrate  concen- 
trations exist,  then  a  longer  period  would  be  required.  Brown  and 
Feldberg  also  found  that  O.ly  acetylcholine  was  liberated  from  a  gang- 
lion weighing  12.9  mg.  and  perfused  with  eserinized  Lockes'  solution, 
during  the  first  5  minutes  of  preganglionic  stimulation  at  17  per  second. 
According  to  Glick,  if  the  substrate  concentration  were  maximal,  78 
milliseconds  would  be  necessary  to  hydrolyze  this  amount,  or  1.5  milli- 
seconds for  splitting  the  acetylcholine  liberated  by  one  nerve  impulse. 
These  times  will  be  longer,  if  lower  substrate  and  enzyme  concentra- 
tions are  assumed.  The  time  for  hydrolysis,  at  the  minimum  rate, 
was  8  seconds,  and  localization  of  enzyme  and  substrate  within  the 
ganglion  cell  would  have  to  be  assumed,  to  explain  enzymatic  destruc- 
tion of  acetylcholine  liberated  by  nerve  impulses  within  the  span  of  the 
refractory  period  of  2  milliseconds. 

EFFECT  OF  pH  AND  ELECTROLYTES  ON 
CHOLINESTERASE  ACTIVITY 

The  effect  of  pH  on  enzyme  activity  is,  of  course,  well  known.  Bern- 
heim  &  Bernheim,'^  employing  a  pharmacological  procedure,  found  a 
pH  optimum  of  8.4  for  the  serum  and  brain  cholinesterases  of  some 
lower  animals.  Glick'-''  ^^  found  practically  this  same  value,  8.4  to 
8.5  for  the  cholinesterases  of  human  serum,  pig's  gastric  mucosa,  horse 
serum,  and  cat  brain.  Calculations  from  the  shape  of  the  pH  activity 
curve,  for  these  various  cholinesterases,  show  that  the  activity  at  7.4 
is  about  65  to  70  per  cent  that  of  the  activity  at  optimal  pH.  A  some- 
what lower  optimal  pH,  7.5-8.0,  has  been  reported  for  red  cell  cholin- 
csterase." 

The  effect  of  various  ions  on  the  activity  of  cholinesterase  has  been 
studied  by  several  groups  of  investigators.  The  activating  effect  of 
Ca"'"'  and  Mg"''"  is  well  established  and,  except  perhaps  for  the  magni- 
tudes of  the  degree  of  activation,  appears  independent  of  the  source  of 
the  enzyme.  Thus,  Nachmansohn^-'  found  that  4  X  10"^  M  Ca""""  in- 
creased the  activity  of  dialyzcd  Torpedo  electric  organ  cholinesterase 
5-fold,  and  4  X  10"^  Mg"'^  increased  the  activity  about  8-  to  9-fold. 
Massart  and  Du  Fait-"  found' that  horse  serum,  which  lost  40  per  cent 
of  its  cholinesterase  activity,  on  dialysis,  regained  its  normal  activity 
in  the  presence  of  2  X  10-'  M  Mg++  or  Ca++.  Mn++  has  been  found  to 
activate   considerably    both    dialyzed   Torpedo   electric   organ   cholin- 


BOD  AN  SKY:  CHOLINESTERASE  525 

esterase  and  dialyzed  horse  serum  cholinesterase.     Ba"^"^  activates  Tor- 
pedo cholinesterase,  but  not  dialyzed  horse  serum  cholinesterase. 

There  has  been  considerable  disagreement,  regarding  the  activating 
effects  of  Na+  and  K"".  But  here,  as  in  other  respects,  these  disagree- 
ments are  resolved,  if  it  is  recognized  that  the  studies  have  been  carried 
out  on  cholinesterases  from  various  sources.  Thus,  1  X  10~^  MK"" 
inhibits  Mendel  and  Rudney's  purified  horse  serum  cholinesterase  40 
per  cent  at  a  substrate  concentration  of  .0015  M  acetylcholine,  15  per 
cent  at  .03  M.^^  Glick-^  found,  at  .02  M  substrate  concentration, 
that  Na+  and  K+  did  not  have  any  regular  effect  on  horse  serum  cholin- 
esterase activity,  but  both  increased  rabbit  serum  cholinesterase  activ- 
ity 25  per  cent  at  0.3  M  Na+  or  K+,  35  per  cent  at  0.5  M,  and  40  per 
cent  at  1.0  M.  Nachmansohn^^  reported  that  both  Na""  and  K+  activ- 
ated Torpedo  cholinesterase. 

There  appears  to  be  a  relationship  between  the  effect  of  Na*  and  K"" 
and  the  effect  of  substrate  concentration  on  reaction  velocity.  Thus, 
in  human  serum  where  the  substrate-reaction  velocity  relationship  fol- 
lows the  Michaelis-Menten  formulation,  0.16  M  NaCl  inhibited  the 
cholinesterase  about  10  to  20  per  cent,  at  substrate  concentrations 
ranging  from  1  X  lO"**  to  4  X  10~*.  On  the  other  hand,  among  red  cell 
cholinesterases,  in  which  reaction  velocity  decreases  at  higher  substrate 
concentrations  (Haldane  formulation),  0.16  M  NaCl,  inhibited  at  low 
substrate  concentrations,  activated  at  substrate  concentrations  greater 
than  about  1  X  10"^'^  M.  Alles'  and  Hawes'  results"  show,  and  Mendel 
and  Rudney^'^  have  emphasized  this  point  particularly,  that,  for  those 
cholinesterases  which  follow  the  Haldane  formulation  for  the  relation- 
ship between  substrate' concentration  and  reaction  velocity,  the  presence 
of  Na"^  or  K""  not  only  increases  the  value  of  the  optimal  reaction  rate, 
but  also  shifts  the  optimum  to  higher  substrate  concentrations. 

So  far  as  anions  are  concerned,  cyanide  has  been  found  to  have  no 
effect  on  horse  or  human  serum  cholinesterase.  Oxalate,  fluoride,  and 
citrate  inhibit  dialyzed  horse  serum  cholinesterase  to  the  extent  of  30 
per  cent  at  0.002  M  concentrations  of  these  ions,  60  per  cent  at 
0.02  M.^" 

INHIBITION  OF  CHOLINESTERASE  ACTIVITY 

■  The  inhibiting  effects  of  various  compounds  on  enzyme  activity  have 
been  generally  formulated  in  terms  of  an  inactive,  but  reversible, 
enzyme  inhibitor  complex.^'  ^     If  the  inhibition  is  non-competitive,  then 

v'  K, 


V       K,  +  I 


(3) 


526  ANNALS  NEW  YORK  ACADEMY  OF  SCIENCES 

Where  v'  is  the  velocity  in  the  presence  of  the  inhibitor,  v  in  its  ab- 
sence, I  is  the  concentration  of  the  inhibitor,  and  Kj  is  the  dissociation 
constant  of  the  enzyme-inhibitor  complex. 

If  the  inhibition  is  competitive,  then  the  following  expression  holds: 

^  +  A'/  1  + 


(-i) 


where  the  terms  have  the  meanings  as  described  in  equations  1  and  3. 
Kj  may  be  calculated  from  the  value  of  the  dissociation  constant,  K's, 
in  the  presence  of  a  constant  concentration  of  inhibitor,  as  follows: 

K:  =  ^r^—  (4') 

The  Michaelis-Menten  derivation  is  based  on  the  assumption  that 
the  concentration  of  enzyme  centers  is  constant  and,  as  compared  with 
the  concentration  of  any  substance  with  which  it  could  combine,  so 
small  that  it  may  be  neglected.  Recently,  Straus  and  Goldstein," 
elaborating  upon  the  ideas  of  Easson  and  Stedman,^^  have  submitted 
a  more  general  formulation  for  the  effect  of  an  inhibitor,  which  takes 
into  account  those  possibilities  in  which  the  concentration  of  enzyme 
centers  may  not  be  negligible.  In  the  presence  of  a  large  excess  of 
substrate, 

/  =         ^.         +  iE     .  (5) 

1  —  I 

total  inhibitor  free  inhibitor  bound  inhibitor 
where  I  is  the  concentration  of  total  inhibitor,  combined  and  free ;  i  is 
the  fraction  of  total  enzyme  combined  with  inhibitor;  E  is  the  concen- 
tration of  total  enzyme;  and  Kj  is  the  dissociation  constant  of  the 
enzyme-inhibitor  complex.  When  the  above  equation  is  divided  by  Kj, 
the  following  expression  is  obtained: 

/'  -  ^.  +  iE'  (6) 

where  /'  =  I/Kj  and  E'  =  E/Kj. 

Simplifications  of  these  equations  are  possible,  under  conditions 
where  E'  is  very  small,  or  very  large,  so  that  the  other  term  on  the  right 
hand  side  of  the  equation  may  be  neglected. 

The  implication  of  equation  6  is  that  the  degree  of  inhibition  de- 
pends upon  the  value  of  £",  namely,  the  ratio  of  the  concentration  of 
enzyme  centers  to  the  dissociation  constant  of  the  complex.  This  may 
be  illustrated  by  taking  values  from  a  theoretical  plot  by  Straus  and 


BOD  AN  SKY:  CHOLINESTERASE  527 

Goldstein.     Thus,  the  log  values  of  /',  at  i  =  0.5  (50  per  cent  inhibi- 
tion) are  as  follows,  for  various  values  of  E': 

E'  log  r 

0.1  0.00 

10  0.79 

100  1.71 

1000  2.70 

It  may,  thus,  be  seen  that  dilution  of  the  enzyme  influences  the  £" 
value,  and  hence,  the  extent  of  inhibition. 

The  values  of  Kj  and  E  may  be  calculated  from  the  experimental 
determination  of  the  inhibition,  i,  at  various  concentrations,  /,  of  in- 
hibitor.    Equation  5  may  be  transposed,  as  follows: 

l  =  KjX  j^.  +  E  (7) 

A  plot  of  -  against  :j -.  should,  therefore,  yield  a  straight  line,  the 

slope  of  which  would  be  Kj  and  the  intercept  on  the  F-axis  would  be  E. 
Goldstein''  has  also  developed  an  expression  for  competitive  equilib- 
rium between  enzyme,  substrate,  and  inhibitor: 


/' 


total  free  combined 

Where  /'  =  I/Kr,  S'  =  S/Ks,  E'l  =  E/Ki, 

Es    =  E/Ks,  and  a  =  1  —  z  or  fractional  activity  of  the  enzyme. 

Another  type  of  derivation  is  possible,  if  E{,  the  amount  of  free 
enzyme,  is  considered  negligible  in  comparison  with  the  amount  of 
enzyme  combined  with  inhibitor  and  substrate.     Then: 


/'     =iS'-aEs')l- -]^{l-a)Er  (9) 


total  free  combined. 

Various  simplifications  of  equations  8  or  9  are  possible,  depending 
upon  whether  we  assume  Ej'  or  Eg-  to  be  small  enough  to  be  neglected, 
or  so  large  that  other  terms  not  involving  them  become  negligible. 

The  investigations  on  in  vitro  inhibition  may  now  be  summarized. 
In  TABLE  2,  are  shown  those  results  which  have  been  formulated,  in 
terms  of  the  dissociation  constants  of  an  inhibitor-enzyme  complex,  in 
accordance  with  the  equations  already  discussed.  Several  points  of 
interest  may  be  noted.  First,  the  dissociation  constants  of  the  enzyme 
complexes  of  physostigmine  and  prostigmine  are  of  a  low  order  of  mag- 
nitude, about  10"^  to  10"^,  as  compared  with  the  dissociation  constants 
of  the  cholinesterase-morphine  derivative  complexes,  10"^  to  10~*.     The 


528 


ANNALS  NEW  YORK  ACADEMY  OF  SCIENCES 


values  of  Eadie^  seem  extraordinarily  low,  but  these  dissociation  con- 
stants have  been  calculated  on  the  assumption  that  one  molecule  of 
enzyme  combines  with  two  of  inhibitor.  Goldstein*'  has  discussed  this 
point  and  has  claimed  that  Eadie's  method  of  determining  reaction 
velocity  involved  a  constant  error.  The  titration  of  released  acetic 
acid,  for  twenty  minutes,  immediately  following  the  addition  of  enzyme 

Table  2 

Dissociation  Constants  of  Cholinesterase-Inhibitoh  Complexes 

Dissociation  constants  calculated  by  method  of  Michaelis-Menten,  except  for 
Straus  &  Goldstein  values.    Values  for  Ki  at  37-38° . 


Inhibitor 

Source  of 
cholinesterase 

Investigator 

Dissociation 
constant,  Ki 

Physostigmine 

Physostigmine 

Physostigmine 

Prostigmine 

Morphine 

Morphine 

Dilaudid 

Codeine 

Desomorphine 

Horse  serum,  22% 
Dog  serum,  4.54% 
Dog  serum 
Dog  serum 
Human  serum 
Dog  serum 
Human  serum 
Human  serum 
Human  serum 

Straus  &  Goldstein22 

Goldstein^ 

Eadie' 

Eadie' 

Wright  &  Sabine* 

Eadie' 

Wright  &  Sabine* 

Wright  &  Sabine* 

Wright  &  Sabine* 

3.7  X  10-* 
3.11  X  10-* 

3       X  10-'*  (n  =  2) 
2       xlO-iMn  =  2) 

8.1  X  10-* 
14.6    X  10-* 

1.2  X  10-3 
4.2    X  10-* 

1.8  X  10-* 

to  a  substrate-inhibitor  mixture,  corresponded  to  a  stage  of  the  reaction 
where  equilibrium  had  not  yet  been  established,  and  where  the  reaction 
velocities  were  higher  than  at  equilibrium.  This  discrepancy  was 
greatest  for  concentrations  of  inhibitor  producing  moderate  inhibitions. 

A  second  point  of  interest  is  the  difference  in  the  dissociation  con- 
stants of  the  complexes  of  the  same  inhibitor  with  cholinesterases  of 
different  sources.  Although  the  difference  is  not  marked,  it  may  be 
seen  that  the  constants  for  the  morphine  complex  are  8.1  X  10~*  for 
human  serum  cholinesterase,  and  14.6  X  10"*  for  dog  serum  cholin- 
esterase. The  occurrence,  among  different  cholinesterases,  of  differing 
sensitivities  to  inhibition  by  the  same  compound,  will  be  discussed 
further. 

In  TABLE  3,  are  shown  the  concentrations  of  cholinesterase  inhibitors 
giving  50  per  cent  of  the  uninhibited  cholinesterase  activity.  It  will 
be  readily  recognized  that  these  values  are  equivalent  to  the  values  of 
the  dissociation  constants,  calculated  by  the  Michaelis-Menten  ex- 
pression for  non-competitive  equilibrium.  This  table  illustrates  the 
order  of  inhibition  of  different  compounds.  It  may  be  seen  that  human 
serum,  rabbit  serum,  and  human  muscle  cholinesterases  are  less  sensi- 
tive to  inhibition  by  physostigmine  than  is  the  horse  serum  cholin- 


BODANSKY:  CHOLIN ESTERASE 


529 


Table  3 

Concentrations  op  Cholinesterase  Inhibitors  Giving  50  Per  Cent  of 

Uninhibited  Velocity 


Inhibitor 

Source  of 
Cholinesterase 

Investigator 

Concentration 
of  Inhibitor 

Physostigmine 

Horse  serum,  4.5% 

Collier  &  AUen^^ 

2.5 

M 
X  10-« 

Physostigmine 

Horse  serum,  22.2% 

Ellis,  Plachte,  &  Straus^^ 

5 

X  10-« 

Physostigmine 

Horse  serum,  11.1% 

Ellis,  Plachte,  &  Straus^^ 

2.5 

X  10-« 

Physostigmine 

Human  serum,  2.5% 

Mazur  &  Bodanskv^^ 

2 

X  10-^ 

Physostigmine 

Rabbit  serum,  12.5% 

Mazur  &  Bodansky-* 

1.2 

X  10-^ 

Physostigmine 

Human  muscle 

Mazur  &  Bodansky^" 

8 

X  10-^ 

Methylene  Bhie 

Horse  serum 

Collier  &  Allen^^ 

1.2 

X  10-« 

Methylene  Blue 

Horse  serum 

Massart  &  DuFait" 

6 

X  10-« 

Acriflayine 

Horse  serum 

Collier  &  Allen^^ 

6.6 

X  10-^ 

Phenothiazone 

Horse  serum 

Collier  &  Allen^^ 

6.7 

X  10-^ 

Thionin 

Horse  serum 

Massart  &  DuFait" 

11 

X  10-' 

Thiamin 

Horse,  rat  serum 

Glick^s 

1.8 

X  10-3 

Isopropyl  antipyrin 

Guinea  pig  serum 

Zeller^s 

2 

X  10-3 

Isopropyl  antipyrin 

Horse  serum 

Zeller" 

0.92 

!  X  10-3 

esterase.  The  basic  dyes,  methylene  blue,  acriflavine,  phenothiazone, 
and  thionin,  are  somewhat  less  potent  inhibitors  than  physostigmine. 
Next,  in  order  of  potency  of  inhibition,  are  thiamin  and  the  antipyrines. 
Table  4  shows  that  several  antipyrines  at  a  concentration  of  3  X  10-^  M 
inhibit  human  serum  cholinesterase  from  30  to  80  per  cent.  Sulfon- 
amides at  this  concentration  inhibit  only  slightly.  Acid  dyes  {e.g., 
Congo  red)  and  p-phenylenediamine  have  been  reported  to  have  no 
inhibiting  action  at  2  X  10"*  M.-' 


Table  4 

Inhibition   of  Human   Serum   Cholinesterase  Activity  by  Antipyrines  and 

Sulfonamides 
(Zeller) 


Inhibitor 

Concentration 
M. 

Inhibition 
per  cent 

Antipyrine 
Aminoantipyrine 
Dimethylaminoantipyrine 
Isopropylantipyrine 

Sulfanilamide 
Irgamid 
Sulfathiazole 
Benzyl  sulfanilamide 

3     X  10-3 
3     X  10-3 
3     X  10-3 
3     X  10-3 

2.5  X  10-3 
2.5  X  10-3 
2.5  X  10-3 
2.5  X  10-3 

51 
30 
38 
80 

-1 

13 

18 

9 

530 


ANNALS  NEW  YORK  ACADEMY  OF  SCIENCES 


It  is,  of  course,  always  of  interest  to  attempt  a  correlation  between 
chemical  structure  of  inhibitors  and  enzyme  action.  In  general,  com- 
pounds which  resemble  the  substrate,  chemically,  inhibit  enzyme  action, 
presumably  by  combining  with  the  same  chemical  groupings  on  the 
enzyme  molecule.  However,  it  must  be  stressed  that  other  chemical 
groups  on  the  inhibitor  molecule  may  influence  this  combination.  The 
evaluation  of  this  influence  requires  considerable  experimentation.  In 
the  case  of  cholinesterase,  relatively  few  data  are  available. 

Acetylcholine  is  a  quaternary  ammonium  compound.  A  survey  of 
the  inhibitors  which  we  have  discussed  shows  that,  although  no  strict 
correlation  can  be  drawn,  between  structure  and  degree  of  inhibition, 
the  closer  the  inhibitor  comes  to  possessing  a  completely  alkylated 
nitrogen  grouping,  the  greater  is  its  inhibition.  Thus,  prostigmine,  one 
of  the  most  effective  inhibitors,  has  a  quaternary  ammonium  grouping. 
Physostigmine  has  two  tertiary  amine  groupings.  Methylene  blue, 
which  is  also  a  potent  inhibitor,  may  be  considered  to  have  a  cjuater- 
nary  ammonium  grouping.  Thionin,  which  is  the  unalkylated  con- 
gener of  methylene  blue,  is  a  less  powerful  inhibitor;  about  20-100 
times  as  great  a  concentration  is  needed  to  produce  50  per  cent 
inhibition. 

That  an  alkylated  amino  grouping  is  important  in  inhibition,  seems 
to  be  generally  true.  Thus,  the  pyrazolone  derivatives  (antipyrines), 
morphine,  caffeine,  percaine,  are  all  moderate  inhibitors.  Unalkylated 
amino  groupings  tend  to  make  a  compound  a  poor  inhibitor:  for  exam- 
ple, diphenylamine  does  not  inhibit  at  2  X  10"*  M.  Acid  groupings 
(COOH,  SO3,  H,  OH),  apparently  tend  to  negate  the  inhibiting  power 
of  alkylated  amino  groups.  Examples  of  this  occur  in  the  sulfon- 
amides and  the  acid  dyes. 


CH3 


CH3         O 

\      ^ 
N-C-0- 

/ 


CH, 


/ 


-N\ 


CH3NH-C-O 


SO4 


CH3 
CH3 


CH3 


CH, 


prostigmine 
S 


CH3    CH3 
physostigmine 


N' 


w 

N 
methylene  blue 


H2N 


BOD  AN  SKY:  CHOLINESTERASE 


531 


CH= 
0=C 


=C.CH3 
N.CH3 
N— CeHs 


O 


H 


/ 


C2H5 


C-N-CH2-CH2-N 
-0C4HC, 


vv 

N 


antipyrine 
CH3-N C=0 


percame 


CHs 


CH 


H2N 


-<^       \-SO2-NH2 


caffeine 


sulfanilamide 


It  must  be  noted,  however,  that  the  relationship  between  the  degree 
of  inhibition  and  the  chemical  structure  of  the  inhibitors  also  depends 
upon  the  source  of  the  cholinesterase.     Table  5  shows  that  percaine, 

Table  5 

Effect  of  Drugs  on  Inhibition  of  Various  Cholinesterases 
(Zeller  and  Bissegger") 


Concentration 
M 

Per  cent  inhibition 

Inhibitor 

Human 
serum 

Human 
red  cells 

Human 
brain 

Percaine 
Irgamid 

Isopropyl  antipyrin 
Morphine 
Caffeine 

6  X  10-3 
6  X  10-3 
6  X  10-3 
6  X  10-3 
6  X  10-3 

94 
46 
81 
66 
4 

25 
4 
26 
76 
42 

IS 
3 

66 
40 

irgamid,  and  isopropyl  antipyrine  inhibit  human  serum  cholinesterase 
much  more  markedly  than  human  red  cell  or  human  brain  cholin- 
esterase. In  contrast,  morphine  inhibits  these  three  cholinesterases  to 
about  the  same  extent,  and  caffeine  inhibits  human  serum  cholinesterase 
only  slightly,  as  compared  with  its  effect  on  human  red  cell  and  human 
brain  cholinesterase.  Nachmansohn  and  Schneeman^"  have  observed 
that  caffeine  inhibits  nervous  tissue  cholinesterases  much  more  mark- 
edly than  those  of  horse  serum  and  guinea  pig  pancreas.  In  contrast, 
the  inhibitions  of  these  latter  cholinesterases  by  quinine,  quinidine, 
cocaine,  and  lobeline  are  more  marked  than  those  of  nervous  tissue. 

In  addition  to  the  inhibitors  which  we  have  discussed  above,  a  num- 
ber of  other  substances  have  been  reported  to  possess  an  inhibiting  ac- 


532  ANNALS  NEW  YORK  ACADEMY  OF  SCIENCES 

tion:  vitamin  K,  ether,  chloral,  and  some  hormone  preparations,  such 
as  estrone,  progestin,  testosterone,  etc.^^-  ^^  Observations  on  these  in- 
hibitions are  not  yet  detailed,  and  their  exact  significance  is  not  yet 
defined.  It  should  be  recognized  that,  in  general,  any  enzyme  is  sub- 
ject to  in  vitro  inhibition  by  a  great  number  of  compounds,  particularly 
at  relatively  high  concentration.  Such  inhibitions  probably  do  re- 
flect an  interaction  of  the  inhibitor  with  the  enzyme  molecule,  but  the 
in  vivo  significance  of  such  inhibitions  is  very  often  cjuestionable. 

There  is  evidence  to  indicate  that  the  inhibitors  which  we  have  al- 
ready described  form  reversible  enzyme-inhibitor  complexes.  Matt- 
hes,"  for  example,  first  showed  that  dialysis  of  a  mixture  of  cholin- 
esterase  and  physostigmine  resulted  in  the  restoration  of  the  enzyme 
activity.  Similarly,  dilution  of  cholinesterase  physostigmine  mixtures 
results  in  a  relative  increase  of  cholinesterase  activity,  presumably  as 
the  result  of  the  dissociation  of  the  inactive  enzyme-inhibitor  com- 
plgx.22,  26  Zeller  has  shown  that  the  inhibition  of  cholinesterase  by 
pyrazolons  and  sulfonamides  is  similarly  reversible,  by  dialysis  of  the 
corresponding  enzyme-inhibitor  complexes. ^^ 

IN  VITRO  AND  IN  VIVO  INHIBITION  BY  DIISOPROPYL- 
FLUOROPHOSPHATE  (DFP) 

We  should  now  like  to  present  a  description  of  the  properties  of  a 
compound,  typical  of  an  entire  group,  which,  in  contrast  to  the  inhibi- 
tors we  have  described  above,  forms  a  combination  with  cholinesterase 
which  it  has,  so  far,  not  been  found  possible  to  reverse.  This  com- 
pound is  diisopropyl-fluorophosphate.  It  is  one  of  a  group  of  alkyl 
fluorophosphates  first  described  by  Lange  and  Krueger.^"*  During  the 
war,  it  was  regarded  as  a  potential  chemical  warfare  agent,  and  its 
properties  were  first  investigated  by  British  workers.  Adrian,  Mc- 
Combie,  B.  A.  Kilby,  and  M.  Kilby^^-  ^"^  noted  the  similarity  between 
the  cholinergic  effects  of  the  fluorophosphates  and  those  of  physostig- 
mine. Mackworth"  found  that  incubation  of  the  alkyl  fluorophos- 
phates with  horse  serum  cholinesterase  resulted  in  the  inactivation  of 
the  enzyme,  and  that  dialysis  of  the  fluorophosphate-cholinesterase 
mixture  did  not  result  in  any  restoration  of  cholinesterase  activity. 
Our  interest  in  the  mechanism  of  the  anticholinesterase  action  was  first 
aroused  when  we  noted  that,  upon  exposure  of  men  to  very  low  concen- 
trations of  this  agent,  the  serum  cholinesterase  was  very  markedly  re- 
duced to  2  to  5  per  cent  of  the  pre-exposure  value,  in  spite  of  the  fact 
that  there  were  only  slight  or  doubtful  systemic   symptoms.     The 


BOD  AN  SKY:  CHOLINESTERASE 


533 


in  vitro  and  in  vivo  inactivation  of  cholinesterase  by  DFP  has  been 
studied  by  Mazur  and  Bodansky,^®  and  the  results  of  these  studies  will 
now  be  briefly  described. 


In  Vitro  Inhibition  of  Cholinesterase  Activity  by  Diisopropyl-Fluoro- 
phosphate  and  by  Physostigmine.  The  inhibition  of  the  activities  of 
serum,  red  cell,  muscle,  and  brain  cholinesterases  of  the  rabbit,  monkey, 
and  man  were  determined,  at  various  concentrations  of  DFP  and 
physostigmine.  In  order  to  obtain  a  general  measure  of  the  extent  of 
inhibition  of  the  different  cholinesterases,  the  relative  velocities  were 
plotted  against  the  negative  logarithm  of  the  molar  concentration  of 
DFP  or  physostigmine.  The  negative  log  molar  concentration  at 
which  50  per  cent  inhibition  occurred,  was  termed  the  pCi  value. 
These  values  for  various  enzyme  preparations  are  shown  in  table  6. 

Table  6 
Sensitivity  of  Various  Cholinesterases  to  Inhibition  by  Diisopropyl-Fluoro- 

PHOSPHATE  and   PhYSOSTIGMINE 

The  values  are  expressed  in  terms  of  the  negative  log  of  the  concentration  of  in- 
hibitor required  to  produce  a  oO  per  cent  inhibition  of  cholinesterase  activity  (pCi). 


Serum 


Red  cells 


Muscle 


Brain 


Diisopropyl-fl  uorophosphate 


Rabbit 

4.1 

5.2 

5.5 

Monkey  {M.  rhesus) 

7.8 

5.5 

5.5 

Human 

7.7 

5.4 

5.4 

6.0 

Horse 

8.3 

Horse*  (purified) 

8.1 

Physostigmine 


*  Horse  serum   cholinesterase   (purified),  prepared  by  Drs.  Northrop  and  Kunitz,  according  to 
directions  of  Mendel  and  Rudney. 

It  may  be  seen  that,  of  the  various  serum  cholinesterases  studied, 
that  of  the  rabbit  was  least  sensitive  to  inhibition  by  DFP.  Thus,  a 
negative  log  molar  concentration  of  4.1  of  DFP  was  necessary  for  50 
per  cent  inhibition  of  rabbit  serum  cholinesterase,  whereas  concentra- 
tions of  about  one  ten  thousandth  as  much  (negative  log  molar  values 
of  7.7  to  8.3)  gave  50  per  cent  inhibition  of  monkey,  human,  and  horse 
serum  cholinesterase  activity.    The  various  red  cell  cholinesterases 


534  ANNALS  NEW  YORK  ACADEMY  OF  SCIENCES 

showed  approximately  the  same  degree  of  sensitivity  toward  inhibition 
by  DFP  (pCi  values  of  5.2  to  5.5),  whereas  human  brain  cholinesterase 
(pCi  =  6.0)  was  somewhat  more  sensitive  than  monkey  or  rabbit  brain 
chohnesterase  (pCi  =  5.5)  to  inhibition  by  DFP.  Purified  cholin- 
esterase of  the  electric  eel  gave  a  pCi  value  of  4.1. 

It  is  of  interest  to  compare  the  sensitivities  of  the  various  tissue 
cholinesterases  from  one  species  to  inhibition  by  DFP.  Rabbit  brain 
and  red  cell  cholinesterases  showed  a  greater  sensitivity  than  did  serum 
cholinesterase;  brain  showed  the  greatest  sensitivity.  Thus,  a  50  per 
cent  inhibition  of  serum  cholinesterase  activity  occurred  at  a  negative 
log  of  the  molar  concentration  of  DFP  of  4.1,  whereas  the  same  degree 
of  red  cell  and  brain  cholinesterase  inhibition  occurred  at  values  of  5.2 
and  5.5,  respectively.  Monkey  serum  cholinesterase  was  much  more 
sensitive  to  inhibition  by  DFP  than  red  cell  or  brain  cholinesterase. 
Human  serum  cholinesterase  was  much  more  sensitive  to  inhibition  by 
DFP  than  human  red  cell  or  brain  cholinesterase.  This  picture  is 
similar  to  that  found  in  the  monkey,  and  is  in  marked  contrast  to  that 
observed  in  the  rabbit. 

Table  6  also  shows  the  sensitivities  of  rabbit  serum,  human  serum, 
and  muscle  cholinesterases  to  inhibition  by  physostigmine.  It  may 
be  seen  that  rabbit  serum  cholinesterase  was  more  sensitive  to 
inhibition  by  physostigmine  (pCi  =  5.9)  than  by  DFP  (pCi  =  4.1), 
whereas  the  reverse  was  true  with  human  serum  cholinesterase. 

The  possibility  existed  that  the  differences  in  sensitivity  of  different 
cholinesterases  to  inhibition  by  DFP  were  due  to  materials,  other  than 
the  enzymes  themselves,  present  in  the  preparations.  Table  6  shows 
that  a  purified  horse  serum  cholinesterase  preparation  had,  within 
experimental  error,  the  same  pCi  value  as  horse  serum  itself.  Heat- 
inactivated  extracts  of  one  tissue,  added  to  a  tissue  possessing  cholin- 
esterase activity,  did  not  alter  the  sensitivity  of  the  latter  to  inhibition 
by  DFP.  Thus,  human  brain  extract  was  heated  to  destroy  its  cholin- 
esterase activity,  and  added  to  human  serum.  The  pCi  value  for  the 
mixture  was  7.7,  the  same  as  that  found  for  human  serum  cholinesterase 
itself. 

In  Vivo  Inhibition  of  Cholinesterase  Activity  by  DFP.  The  extent 
to  which  various  cholinesterases  are  inhibited  in  vivo,  after  administra- 
tion of  DFP,  may  be  considered  to  depend,  not  only  on  the  sensitivity 
of  the  particular  tissue  cholinesterase  to  inhibition  by  DFP,  but  also 
on  the  localization  and,  hence,  of  the  concentration  of  DFP  in  the  tissue. 
In  rabbits  exposed  to  DFP  vapor,  severe  muscular  tremors  and  death 


BODANSKY:  CHOLINESTERASE 


535 


occurred  at  the  higher  exposures;  at  lower  exposures,  no  symptoms  ex- 
cept miosis  occurred.  In  most  instances,  the  decrease  in  red  blood  cell 
cholinesterase  activity  was  greater  than  that  in  serum  eholinesterase 
activity.  It  will  be  recalled  that  in  vitro  rabbit  red  cell  cholinesterase 
is  more  sensitive  than  serum  cholinesterase  to  inhibition  by  DFP. 
When  rhesus  monkeys  were  exposed  to  DFP,  the  plasma  cholinesterase 
activity  was  decreased  to  only  1  to  5  per  cent  of  normal,  at  almost  all 
exposures,  whereas  the  red  cell  cholinesterase  activity  showed  only 
slight  decreases  at  the  lower  exposures.  This  marked  difference  in  the 
extent  of  decrease  paralleled  the  considerable  in  vitro  difference  be- 
tween the  sensitivities  of  monkey  red  cell  and  serum  cholinesterases  to 
inhibition  by  DFP. 

The  effect  of  intravenously  injected  DFP  on  cholinesterase  activity, 
in  the  rabbit,  is  shown  in  table  7.     It  can  be  seen  that,  in  most  in- 


Table  7 

The  Effect  of  Intravenously  Injected  Diisopropyl-Fluorophosphate  on  Rabbit 
Plasma,  Red  Cells,  and  Brain  Cholinesterase  Activity  In  Vivo 


Blood 

sample 

time 

Relative  cholinesterase 

Animal 

Dose 
mg. 

activity* 

T?pmnrkK 

number 

per  kg. 

min. 

Plasma 
per  cent 

Red  cells 
per  cent 

Brain 
per  cent 

327 

3.0 

4 

0 

0 

0 

Died  immediately 

326 

0.3 

23 

15 

7 

12 

Muscle  tremors 

328 

0.3 

26 

5 

0 

5 

Muscle  tremors 

491 

0.1 

26 

54 

41 

59 

No  symptoms 

492 

0.1 

25 

37 

29 

57 

No  symptoms 

330 

0.05 

27 

60 

19 

74 

No  symptoms 

331 

0.05 

43 

51 

29 

73 

No  symptoms 

*  These  values  are  per  cent  of  the  pre-exposure  values. 

stances,  the  red  cell  cholinesterase  activity  is  more  markedly  reduced 
than  that  of  the  serum  cholinesterase.  This  difference  corresponds  to 
the  in  vitro  difference  in  sensitivities.  The  brain  cholinesterase  activ- 
ities were  reduced  to  about  the  same  extent  as  the  serum  cholinesterase 
activities  at  the  lower  doses,  and  slightly  less  at  the  higher  doses.  This 
finding  did  not  correspond  to  the  in  vitro  sensitivity  of  brain  cholin- 
esterase to  inhibition  by  DFP.  The  brain  cholinesterase  activities, 
corresponding  to  the  appearance  of  symptoms,  were  less  than  about  60 
per  cent  of  normal. 

The  effect  of  intravenous  injections  of  DFP  in  the  monkey  are  shown 
in  TABLE  8.  A  dose  of  0.3  mg.  per  kg.  was  fatal  in  10  minutes.  The 
serum,  red  blood  cell,  and  brain  cholinesterases  were  reduced  to  very 


536 


ANNALS  NEW  YORK  ACADEMY  OF  SCIENCES 


low  or  zero  levels  of  activity.  Essentially  the  same  results  were  ob- 
tained at  doses  of  0.2  and  0.25  mg.  per  kg.,  except  that  the  survival 
period  was  longer.  At  a  dose  of  0.1  mg.  per  kg.,  the  animal  survived, 
although  the  serum  and  red  cell  cholinesterase  activities  were  very  low. 
At  0.02  mg.  per  kg.,  the  serum  cholinesterase  activity  was  reduced  to  a 
very  low  level,  2  per  cent  of  normal,  whereas  the  brain  cholinesterase 
activity  was  decreased  only  slightly,  to  78  per  cent  of  normal.     This 

Table  8 

The  Effect  of  Intravenously  Injected  Diisoproptl-Fluorophosphate  on  Mon- 
key Plasma,  Red  Cells,  and  Brain  Cholinesterase  Activity  In  Vivo 


Animal 
number 

Dose 

mg. 

per.  kg. 

Blood 

sample 
time 
min. 

Relative  cholinesterase 

activity 

Per  cent  of  pre-injedion 

value 

Remarks 

Plasma 

Red  cells 

Brain 

4 
6 
3 

1 
2 

5 

0.3 

0.25 

0.2 

0.1 
0.1 

0.02 

10 
27 
2      hrs. 

24 

3H  hrs. 

60 

0 
0 
0 

0 
0 

2 

2 

1 
0 

2 
10 

14 

0 
0 
0 

0 

78 

Died  in  10  minutes 
Died  in  33  minutes 
Severe  symptoms, 

sacrificed 
No  symptoms 
Muscle  tremors, 

diarrhea 
No  symptoms 

marked  difference  between  the  decreases  of  serum  and  brain  cholin- 
esterase was  similar  to  that  obtained  at  low  exposures  of  monkeys  to 
DFP  vapor,  and  parallels  strikingly  the  in  vitro  difference  in  sensitivity 
between  the  monkey  serum  and  brain  cholinesterases  to  inhibition  by 
DFP.  As  will  be  seen  presently,  these  findings  are  quite  similar  to 
those  obtained  in  man.  Although  the  red  blood  cell  cholinesterase 
was  of  about  the  same  order  of  in  vitro  sensitivity  as  the  brain  cholin- 
esterase, it  was  reduced  in  vivo,  at  the  very  low  dose,  to  a  greater 
degree. 

In  the  monkey,  symptoms  were  absent,  even  when  the  serum  cholin- 
esterase levels  were  zero.  In  most  instances,  the  occurrence  of  severe 
symptoms  or  death  was  associated  with  zero  levels  of  brain  cholin- 
esterase activity.  After  exposure  to,  or  injection  of,  DFP,  the  serum 
cholinesterase  returned  very  slowly  to  normal.  In  the  few  instances  in 
which  this  return  was  followed,  about  50  per  cent  of  the  normal  serum 
cholinesterase  activity  was  regained  in  about  7  days. 

Men  were  also  exposed  to  DFP  vapor.  One  group  of  7  men  was  ex- 
posed to  a  concentration  of  19  micrograms  per  liter,  for  8%  minutes; 


BODANSKY:  CHOLINESTERASE 


537 


a  second  group  of  6,  to  a  concentration  of  27.1  micrograms  per  liter, 
for  9  minutes;  and  2  men,  to  28.8  micrograms  per  liter,  for  10  minutes, 
40  seconds,  and  27  micrograms  per  liter  for  6  minutes,  20  seconds,  re- 
spectively. The  symptoms  were  extremely  mild.  All  of  the  men 
showed  miosis  and  most  of  them  complained  of  a  slight  feeling  of  tight- 
ness in  the  chest,  lasting  for  several  hours.  The  following  symp- 
toms were  observed  occasionally:  increased  nasal  secretion,  nausea, 
salivation. 

Table  9 

Effect  of  Inh.alation   of  Diisopropyl-Fluorophosphate  Vapor  on   Serum 
Cholixesterase  Activity  In  Vivo  in  Max 


■       -B 

Subject 

Concen- 
tration 

of  DFP 
vapor 

Duration 

of 
exposure 

Relative  cholinesterase  activity  at 
various  intervals  after  exposure* 

5-30 
min. 

Days 

1 

2 

3 

4 

6 

8 

10 

15 

17 

y  per  1. 

min. 

4 
3 
2 
3 

1 

13 

7 

14 

12 

7 

29 
28 
3 
31 
30 

58 
53 
55 
50 
52 

71 
77 
72 
69 
68 

J.H. 

R.L. 

W.B. 

M.G. 

J.P.M. 

19 
19 
19 
19 
19 

8.7 
8.7 
8.7 
8.7 
8.7 

*  These  values  are  per  cent  of  the  pre-exposure  values 

Table  9  shows  the  decreases  in  serum  cholinesterase  activity,  imme- 
diately after  exposure,  and  the  rate  of  return  of  the  activity  to  normal. 
It  may  be  seen  that,  immediately  after  gassing,  the  serum  cholinesterase 
activity  decreased  to  about  1  to  5  per  cent  of  the  pre-exposure  value. 
The  rate  of  return  to  normal  was  very  slow.  On  the  average,  the  activ- 
ity returned  to  about  30  per  cent,  in  four  days;  to  about  50  per  cent,  in 
8  days;  and  to  about  70  per  cent,  in  15  days.  The  red  cell  cholin- 
esterase activities  of  several  of  these  men  were  determined  immediately 
after  exposure  and  were  found  to  be  only  slightly  decreased  below  pre- 
exposure values.  These  results  show,  therefore,  a  correlation  with  the 
in  vitro  sensitivities  of  human  serum  and  red  cell  activities. 


Rate  of  Restoration  of  Rabbit  Plasma,  Red  Cell,  and  Brain  Cholin- 
esterase Activities  after  Poisoning  with  DFP.  The  slow  rate  of  re- 
generation of  serum  cholinesterase  activities,  demonstrated  above  in 
man  and  monkey,  raised  the  question  as  to  the  rate  of  regeneration  of 
brain  cholinesterase  activity.  The  average  brain  cholinesterase  activ- 
ity Vas  first  determined  in  a  series  of  normal  rabbits.     Each  of  a  group 


538  ANNALS  NEW  YORK  ACADEMY  OF  SCIENCES 

of  about  50  rabbits  was  injected  with  0.3  mg.  DFP  per  kg.,  and  blood 
samples  were  taken,  before  injection,  for  determination  of  normal 
plasma  and  red  cell  cholinesterase.  At  this  dose,  the  rabbits  developed 
tremors  within  about  15  minutes  after  injection  and  continued  to  have 
these  tremors  throughout  the  day.  About  10  per  cent  of  the  animals 
died.  The  survivijig  animals  were  free  of  symptoms  the  day  following 
injection.  At  suitable  intervals  after  injection,  blood  was  taken  for  de- 
termination of  plasma  and  red  cell  cholinesterase  activities.  At  these 
or  other  times  after  injection,  1  to  6  rabbits  were  selected  for  sacrifice. 
Brains  were  removed,  within  5  minutes  after  death,  and  the  brain 
cholinesterase  activities  determined.  The  plasma  cholinesterase  ac- 
tivity returned  to  normal  values  in  about  5  days.  The  red  cell  cholin- 
esterase activity  returned  to  normal  somewhat  more  slowly.  It  was 
about  50  per  cent  of  normal  in  about  5  days  and  attained  the  normal, 
pre-injection  level  in  10  days.  The  rate  of  recovery  of  brain  cholin- 
esterase activity  was  exceedingly  slow.  Ten  days  after  injection,  the 
brain  cholinesterase  activity  was  about  50  to  60  per  cent  of  normal. 
Twenty  to  thirty  days  after  injection,  it  was  about  60  to  70  per  cent 
of  normal.  Fifty  days  after  injection,  the  brain  cholinesterase  activity 
had  returned  to  90  per  cent  of  normal. 

Attempts  at  Reversal  of  DFP  Inhibition  of  Cholinesterase.  It  has 
been  shown  that  the  inhibitions  of  phosphatase  by  amino  acids,^^  of 
pepsin  by  proteolytic  digestion  products,^''  and  of  cholinesterase  by 
physostigmine^^'  ^^  may  be  reversed  by  subjecting  the  enzyme-inhibitor 
mixture  to  dialysis  or  dilution.  In  the  present  study,  the  serum  and 
brain  extracts  of  rabbits  injected  with  0.3  mg./kg.  DFP  were  dialyzed 
against  several  changes  of  saline,  for  about  24  hours.  Rabbit  plasma 
was  also  treated  in  vitro  with  DFP  and  then  dialyzed,  for  24  hours. 
In  neither  type  of  experiment  was  there  any  increase  in  activity  of  the 
inactivated  cholinesterase.  The  in  vitro  results  are  in  agreement  with 
those  of  Mackworth.^^  Dilution  of  mixtures  of  cholinesterase  and 
fluorophosphate  failed  to  show  any  relative  increase  in  enzyme  activ- 
ity. This  was  in  contrast  to  the  results  obtained  on  dilution  of  physo- 
stigmine-cholinesterase  mixtures. 

In  view  of  the  difference  among  the  cholinesterases  of  different  tis- 
sues to  inhibition  by  DFP,  it  would  be  unjustified  to  draw  any  conclu- 
sions from  our  data  concerning  the  sensitivity  to  inhibition  of  cholin- 
esterases, at  autonomic  effector  organs,  ganglia,  or  myoneural  junctions. 
According  to  the  concept  of  chemical  transmission  of  nervous  impulses, 
the  extent  of  cholinesterase  inhibition,  at  these  sites,  should  be  corre- 


BODANSKY:  CHOLINESTERASE  539 

lated  with  the  appearance  of  various  chohnergic  symptoms.  In  gen- 
eral, in  the  monkey  or  rabbit,  such  cholinergic  symptoms  as  muscular 
tremors,  salivation,  and  diarrhea  were  associated  with  low  red  cell  and 
brain  cholinesterase  activity,  and  death  was  associated  with  zero  brain 
cholinesterase  activity.  However,  this  association  is  to  be  regarded  as 
fortuitous.  Conversely,  it  should  be  emphasized  that  depression  of 
serum  cholinesterase  activity  does  not  necessarily  indicate  the  appear- 
ance of  cholinergic  symptoms.  In  monkey  and  man,  for  example,  the 
sei'um  cholinesterase  activity  may  be  reduced  to  extremely  low  levels, 
without  the  manifestations  of  such  symptoms. 

The  persistence  of  low  serum,  red  cell,  and  brain  cholinesterase  ac- 
tivity in  the  rabbit,  for  periods  of  5,  10,  and  60  days,  respectively,  and 
of  low  serum  cholinesterase  activities  in  the  monkey  and  man,  for 
periods  of  at  least  one  to  two  weeks,  offers  evidence  in  support  of  the 
irreversibility  of  inactivation  in  vivo.  Hall  and  Ettinger'*°  found  that, 
after  injections  of  physostigmine  in  the  dog,  the  serum  cholinesterase 
activity  dropped  to  10  to  25  per  cent  of  normal  in  about  a  half  hour 
and  returned  to  normal  in  two  hours.  This  prompt  restoration  of  nor- 
mal activity  may  well  be  expected  in  the  case  of  a  readily  reversible 
inhibitor-enzyme  complex.  On  the  other  hand,  the  long  periods  of 
time  necessary  for  the  restoration  of  normal  cholinesterase  activity, 
after  exposure  to  DFP  vapor  or  injections  with  DFP  solutions,  in  the 
instances  mentioned,  are  of  the  same  order  of  magnitude  as  those  nec- 
essary for  the  regeneration  of  protein/^  and  would  seem  to  indicate  a 
synthesis  of  enzyme  protein. 

DFP  has  already  been  proved  to  be  of  considerable  value  as  a  tool  in 
investigative  work.  Its  anticholinesterase  action  has  made  it  a  candi- 
date for  clinical  trials  in  Myasthenia  gravis  and  glaucoma.*^"^''  The 
marked  degree  to  which  it  may  inhibit  cholinesterase  activity  in  vivo, 
and  the  character  of  this  inhibition,  have,  as  we  have  seen,  permitted 
studies  of  the  rate  of  regeneration  of  various  tissue  cholinesterases. 
They  open  the  way  to  further  studies  on  the  way  in  which  diet,  drugs, 
or  the  existence  of  various  pathological  lesions  influence  the  regenera- 
tion of  cholinesterase.  We  have  also  seen  that  DFP  has  permitted 
more  incisive  studies  into  the  role  of  cholinesterase  in  nerve  transmis- 
sion."' '^^ 

SPECIFICITY 

Specificity  of  Cholinesterase  Action.  The  question  of  the  specificity 
of  the  cholinesterase  activities  of  various  tissue  extracts  has  claimed 
considerable  attention.     Although  certain  general  distinctions  between 


540  ANNALS  NEW  YORK  ACADEMY  OF  SCIENCES 

the  pattern  of  certain  of  the  cholinesterases  may  be  made,  it  will  be 
shown  that  there  are  a  number  of  exceptions  to  any  strict  classification. 

Easson  and  Stedman^'  proposed  the  following  criteria  for  the  specific- 
ity of  the  cholinesterase  action  of  different  sera:  (a)  relative  action  of 
the  serum  on  choline  and  non-choline  esters;  (b)  inhibition  of  the  ester- 
splitting  action  by  prostigmine;  (c)  hydrolysis  of  mixed  substrates. 
Thus,  human  serum  was  considered  to  contain  a  specific  cholinesterase, 
because  its  action  on  non-choline  esters  was  about  l/80th  of  that  to- 
wards butyrylcholine,  and  because  both  actions  were  inhibited  to  the 
same  extent  by  the  same  concentration  (10"''  M)  of  prostigmine.  On 
the  other  hand,  guinea  pig  serum  was  considered  to  contain  a  specific 
cholinesterase  and  a  non-choline,  ester-splitting  esterase;  because  the 
rate  of  actions  on  butyrylcholine  and  methylbutyrate  were  of  about  the 
same  magnitude;  because  10~^  M  prostigmine  inhibited,  markedly,  only 
the  action  on  butyrylcholine;  and  because  the  actions  on  a  mixture  of 
butyrylcholine  and  methylbutyrate  were  equal  to  the  sum  of  the  action 
on  each.  We  have  found  that  judgment  as  to  the  relative  action  of  a 
serum  or  tissue  extract  on  acetylcholine  and  non-choline  esters  may  de- 
pend considerably  on  the  particular  esters  employed.  For  example,  if 
the  actions  of  human  and  rabbit  brain  extracts  on  acetylcholine  are 
compared  with  that  on  monoacetin,  it  is  found  that  there  is  relatively 
little  hydrolysis  of  the  latter.  It  might  be  concluded  that  these  extracts 
contain,  chiefly,  specific  cholinesterase.  On  the  other  hand,  if  the  ac- 
tion is  compared  with  that  on  triacetin,  it  is  found  that  there  is  consid- 
erable hydrolysis  of  this  ester,  and  it  might  just  as  readily  be  concluded 
that  the  content  of  non-specific  esterase  is  very  high. 

Mendel  and  Rudney^^  stated  that  there  were  two  different  cholin- 
esterases in  the  body :  one  of  which  acted  exclusively  on  choline  esters ; 
and  the  other,  a  non-specific  enzyme,  which  split  both  choline  and  non- 
choline  esters.  These  they  termed  "true"  and  "pseudo"-cholinesterases, 
respectively.  In  addition  to  some  of  the  criteria  for  specificity  em- 
ployed by  Easson  and  Stedman,*^  Mendel  and  Rudney^^  pointed  out 
that  the  non-specific  or  "pseudo"-cholinesterases  exhibited  maximal  ac- 
tivity at  high  concentrations  of  the  substrate,  acetylcholine,  whereas 
the  "true"  cholinesterase  showed  optimal  activity  at  low  substrate  con- 
centrations. According  to  these  authors,  inhibition  of  both  choline 
and  non-choline  ester  hydrolysis  constitutes  a  criterion  for  distinguish- 
ing the  cholinesterase  as  "pseudo."  We  have  found  that  the  anti- 
cholinesterase compound,  DFP,  markedly  inhibits  the  mouse  brain 
hydrolyses  of  both  acetylcholine  and  triacetin.  According  to  the  cri- 
teria of  Mendel  and  Rudney,  this  finding  should  classify  mouse  brain 


BOD  AN  SKY:  CHOLIN  ESTERASE  541 

cholinesterase  as  a  "pseudo"-cholinesterase.  Yet,  these  authors  had 
classified  it  as  a  "true"  cholinesterase,  since  its  hydrolysis  of  acetyl- 
choline was  inhibited  by  physostigmine,  whereas  the  hydrolysis  of  non- 
choline  esters  was  unaffected  by  this  compound. 

According  to  Mendel's  and  Rudney's  criteria,  the  red  cells  of  sev- 
eral species  (human,  horse,  sheep,  ox)  contained  two  enzymes:  a  spe- 
cific or  "true"  cholinesterase  and  a  non-choline  ester-splitting  enzyme. 
Mendel  and  Rudney  classified  red  cell  cholinesterase  as  "true"  cholin- 
esterase, because  a  purified  preparation  hydrolyzed  acetylcholine,  but 
failed  to  hydrolyze  non-choline  esters,  as  exemplified  by  methylbuty- 
rate  or  tributyrin.  We  have  found  that  such  a  purified  preparation 
splits  triacetin,  a  non-choline  ester,  which  Mendel  and  Rudney  did  not 
test.  ■ 

Nachmansohn  and  Rothenberg"  have  inclined  towards  the  view  that 
specificity  is  relative,  and  that  tissue  extracts  containing  the  specific 
cholinesterase  split  acetylcholine  at  a  higher  rate  than  other  esters. 
According  to  these  investigators,  the  esterase  in  all  nerve  tissue  is  either 
exclusively  or  predominantly  cholinesterase.  Results  which  we  have 
obtained  confirm  those  of  Nachmansohn  and  Rothenberg,  except  for 
the  ester  (triacetin) ,  which  was  not  tested  by  these  investigators.  This 
ester  was  hydrolyzed  more  rapidly  than  acetylcholine. 

The  results  on  several  aspects  of  the  action  and  inhibition  of  various 
cholinesterases  have  been  summarized  in  table  10.  Although  the  data 
are  not  complete,  it  may  be  seen  that,  except  in  two  respects,  the  cholin- 
esterases may  be  divided  into  two  general  groups.  In  the  first  group, 
the  enzymes  follow  the  Michaelis-Menten  formulation  for  the  relation- 
ship between  reaction  velocity  and  substrate  concentrations.  There  is 
no  inhibition  by  caffeine ;  a  marked  inhibition  by  percaine ;  no  activa- 
tion by  Na""  and  K+;  and  failure  to  hydrolyze  acetyl-B-methyl  cho- 
line. In  the  second  group,  inhibition  occurs  at  higher  substrate  con- 
centrations; there  are  also:  inhibition  by  caffeine;  slight  inhibition  by 
percaine;  activation  by  Na""  and  K"";  and  the  ability  to  hydrolyze  acetyl- 
B-methyl  chohne.  However,  as  already  pointed  out,  DFP  inhibits  the 
non-choline  ester  hydrolysis  of  the  enzymes,  in  both  of  these  groups. 
Although  DFP  seems  to  inhibit  the  cholinesterases  of  the  first  group 
more  markedly,  there  is  considerable  variation  in  sensitivity  to  inhibi- 
tion. Moreover,  there  is  no  sharp  distinction  between  the  enzymes  of 
these  two  groups,  with  respect  to  the  ratio  of  velocities  at  which  they 
hydrolyze  triacetin  and  acetylcholine.  This  latter  finding  may  be  ex- 
plained by  assuming  that  there  are  varying  amounts  of  non-acetyL: 
choline  hydro lyzing  esterases  in  these  preparations.     Y\\ri\\QYJff^^%  f q 


542  ANNALS  NEW  YORK  ACADEMY  OF  SCIENCES 

indicated  to  explore  the  actions  of  various  cholinesterases,  with  respect 
to  the  criteria  indicated  in  Table  10. 

IN  VIVO  CHANGES  OF  CHOLINESTERASE 

Alterations  of  Cholinesterase  Activity  in  Disease.  Considerable  clin- 
ical and  pharmacological  investigation  into  the  in  vivo  changes  of 
cholinesterase  activity  has  been  conducted,  with  a  view  to  determining 
the  physiological  significance  of  cholinesterase.  The  pathological  con- 
dition which  has  attracted  most  attention,  in  this  respect,  is  Myasthenia 
gravis.  In  this  condition,  which  is  characterized  by  muscle  weakness 
and  inclination  to  fatigue,  it  has  been  postulated  that  there  is  a  defi- 
ciency of  acetylcholine  at  the  neuromuscular  junction.  Such  a  postu- 
lated deficiency  may,  of  course,  be  brought  about  by  a  failure  to  syn- 
thesize acetylcholine,  or  by  an  excessive  amount,  or  excessive  activity, 
of  cholinesterase  at  the  neuromuscular  junctions.  That  the  latter 
mechanism  is  operative  has  been  assumed,  because  .of  the  finding  that 
prostigmine,  an  inhibitor  of  cholinesterase  activity  in  vitro  and  of 
serum  cholinesterase  activity  in  vivo,  results  in  clinical  improvement.*® 
There  is,  however,  no  conclusive  evidence  of  increased  cholinesterase 
concentration  or  activity  at  the  myoneural  junctions.  A  number  of 
investigators**^"^^  have  failed  to  find  increased  cholinesterase  activity 
in  the  serum  in  Myasthenia  gravis,  and,  although  the  cholinesterase 
activity  of  muscle  may  perhaps  not  be  regarded  as  too  specific,  there 
has  been  a  similar  inability  to  find  increase  of  cholinesterase  activity 
in  muscle.^''  Other  explanations  of  the  physiological  fault  in  Myas- 
thenia gravis  and  of  the  action  of  various  drugs  have  been  submitted 
by  Gammon,  Harvey,  and  Masland.^' 

There  are,  however,  several  conditions  in  which  definite  changes  in 
serum  cholinesterase  activity  have  been  reported.  There  is  fairly 
general  agreement  that  debilitating  diseases,  such  as  tuberculosis,  can- 
cer, and  liver  disease,  are  characterized  by  low  serum  cholinesterase 
activities. °®"'^*  For  example,  Faber'^*  found  ranges  of  65  to  150  units  in 
normal  men  and  57  to  184  in  normal  women.  In  acute  hepatitis,  values 
ranging  from  41  to  51  units  were  obtained;  in  liver  cirrhosis,  activities 
ranging  from  77  to  92;  in  cancer,  from  33  to  99;  and  in  uremia,  from 
32  to  56  units.  High  serum  cholinesterase  activities  have  been  re- 
ported in  hyperthyroidism.*^'*'  ®^ 

The  author  does  not  know,  however,  of  any  evidence  to  indicate  that 
the  low  serum  cholinesterase  values  found  in  debilitating  diseases  are 
of  any  special  significance,  so  far  as  transmission  of  nervous  impulses 
is  concerned.     Indeed,  these  low  cholinesterase  activities  appear  to  be 


BOD  AN  SKY:  CHOLINESTERASE 


543 


Ed 

m 

< 

Eh 

g 

o 

w 
o 

M 

o    2 
J    1> 


o 

;?; 
o 

<! 
o 

CO 

H 

CM 


03 

OJt*- 

O        t^  -f 

o 

o      CO 

<M        OO 

lO 

(M        CO 

o     do 

d 

oi      d 

>  ^      ■ 

_^ 

--''>>  a^  >> 

>>j3  d-? 

Acet 

-met 
3holi 
ydrc 

+       OO 

+ 

+     + 

«  -^ 

c 

W.2 

-1-^ 

i~> 

+  0 

+ 

+ 

^•1 

o 

o3 

tc  a; 

•s.s 

>i-;5  a3 

-— c    O    t< 

O 

+ 

+     + 

"-5 

^i- 

!-2 
'-S 

ffi§ 

.S 

Q 

.  .^  ^ 

^  cot^  t^ 

^ 

o 

If 

■*'odi>  N^ 

id 

d 

lO 

4J   C 

c  2 

+ 

9X! 

+ 

+ 

+1 

S'^ 

+ 

(^  c 

*^H 

-,     r^ 

(U     .J 

C   CI 

'S!-^ 

+ 

+ 

o 

+ 

+ 

o^ 

•  .-H 

i  >>c 

s3-*^   O 

cfi    O    ^ 

-Q  "S  "S 

3   >   ^ 

rJ2 

fl 

>>  - 

03 

si  6^ 

a 

3 

03           (C 

S      3 

3x2 

-a  o  £ 

s 

s 

-« 

^ 

« 

s~ 

^ 

05 

oq 

544  ANNALS  NEW  YORK  ACADEMY  OF  SCIENCES 

merely  a  reflection  of  the  state  of  the  serum  proteins.  Faber^''  has 
found  that  there  is  a  direct  proportionality  between  serum  cholin- 
esterase  activities  and  the  concentration  of  serum  albumin,  but  not 
between  these  activities  and  the  concentration  of  total  serum  protein 
or  that  of  the  serum  globulin.  This,  of  course,  does  not  imply  that 
cholinesterase  is  an  albumin.  Faber"^  has  noted  that,  in  proteinurias, 
the  serum  cholinesterase  activities  remain  high,  relative  to  the  concen- 
tration of  the  serum  albumin.  There  is  an  indication,  in  these  observa- 
tions, that  the  formation  of  serum  cholinesterase  parallels  that  of  serum 
albumin. 

We  have  already  discussed  the  effects  of  DFP  on  the  in  vivo  activity 
of  serum,  red  cell,  and  brain  cholinesterases.  There  are,  in  the  litera- 
ture, similar  studies  on  other  drugs.  Perhaps  one  of  the  most  detailed 
is  that  of  Schutz,^^'  *'^'  '°  concerning  the  effect  of  barbiturates.  This 
investigator  noted  that  the  prolonged  administration  of  these  drugs,  in 
man  and  animals,  resulted  in  marked  decreases  of  serum  cholinesterase 
activity,  although  these  drugs  in  concentrations  of  about  0.01  M  do 
not  inhibit  the  in  vitro  serum  cholinesterase  activity.  He  also  ob- 
served that  such  prolonged  administration  in  animals  resulted  in  in  vivo 
decreases  of  brain,  spinal  cord,  and  muscle  cholinesterase  activity.  His 
explanation  was  that  the  barbiturates  decreased  the  activity  of  the 
cholinergic  system  and,  hence,  the  demand  for  cholinesterase.  Such 
an  explanation  is,  of  course,  teleological  and  demands  direct  proof  of 
decreased  synthesis  of  the  cholinesterases  involved. 

CONCLUSION 

As  a  conclusion  to  this  review,  it  may  be  of  value  to  emphasize  certain 
general  points.  The  literature,  as  well  as  our  own  data,  indicates  that 
we  cannot  speak  of  "one"  cholinesterase,  identical  in  its  properties,  no 
matter  where  it  may  be  found.  Within  one  species,  the  enzyme  differs, 
in  certain  respects,  from  tissue  to  tissue,  and  the  enzyme  of  a  given  tis- 
sue may  differ  from  species  to  species.  Perhaps,  then,  it  would  be  more 
proper  to  speak  of  a  "family"  of  cholinesterases,  the  members  of  which 
resemble  each  other  in  some  attributes  and  differ  in  others.  Classifi- 
cation into  certain  groups  may  now  be  possible,  but  even  within  such 
groups,  differences  in  properties  may  occur.  Considerably  more  ex- 
perimental work  with  various  criteria  of  enzyme  action  is  necessary,  in 
order  to  achieve  a  more  satisfactory  classification.  The  writer  believes 
that  many  of  the  controversies  on  the  properties  of  cholinesterase  which 
have  occurred  in  the  literature  will  be  resolved,  if  the  foregoing  consid- 
erations are  kept  in  mind. 


BODANSKY:  CHOLINESTERASE  545 

It  would  also  appear  that  these  considerations  will  be  of  aid  in 
elucidating  various  physiological  and  physiopathological  problems.  It 
has  already  been  pointed  out  that  the  cholinesterase  activity  of  the 
serum  is  not  necessarily  an  indicator  of  the  cholinesterase  activity  of 
the  brain.  Attempts  to  define  the  role  of  cholinesterase  activity  in  the 
transmission  of  nerve  impulses  must  concern  themselves  with  a  study 
of  the  properties  of  cholinesterase,  at  various  loci  of  the  nervous  system, 
and  with  a  correlation  of  the  in  vivo  alterations  of  the  activity  of  these 
cholinesterases,  in  response  to  administration  of  drugs  or  to  other  fac- 
tors which  have  an  effect  on  nerve  activity.  Finally,  it  would  seem 
that  the  compound,  DFP,  because  of  its  capacity  for  decreasing  mark- 
edly, and  apparently  with  a  considerable  degree  of  irreversibility,  the 
content  of  cholinesterase  in  various  tissues,  may  serve  as  a  most  useful 
tool  for  elucidating  the  role  of  cholinesterase  and  the  factors  which 
influence  its  synthesis  and  degradation. 

BIBLIOGRAPHY 

1.  Dale,  H.  H. 

1914.     J.  Pharm.  Exp.  Therap.  6:  147. 

2.  Loewi,  O.,  &  £.  Navratil 

1926.     Pfliig.  Arch.  ges.  Physiol.  214:  678,  689. 

3.  Michaelis,  L.,  &  M.  L.  Menten 

1913.     Biochem.  L.  49:333. 

4.  Linaweaver,  H.,  &  D.  Burk 
1934.     J.  Am.  Chem.  Soc.  56 :  658. 

6.  Haldane,  J.  B.  S. 

1930.     Enzymes.     Longmans,  Green  &  Co.  London,  New  York  and  Toronto.   85. 

6.  Goldstein,  A. 

1944.  J.  Gen.  Physiol.  27:  529. 

7.  Eadie,  G.  S. 

1942.  J.  Biol.  Chem.  146:85. 
1941.    J.  Biol.  Chem.  138:  597. 

8.  Wright,  C.  I.,  &  J.  C.  Sabine 

1943.  J.  Pharm.  Exp.  Therap.  78:  375. 

9.  Glick,  D. 

1937.  Biochem.  J.  31:  521. 

10.  Glick,  D. 

1938.  J.  Gen.  Physiol.  21:  431. 

11.  Alles,  G.  A.,  «fe  R.  C.  Hawes 

1940.     J.  Biol.  Chem.  133:  375. 

12.  Zeller,  E.  A.,  &  A.  Bissegger 
1943.     Helvet.  Chim.  Acta  23:  1619. 

13.  Mendel,  B.,  &  H.  Rudney 
1943.     Biochem.  J.  37:  59. 

14.  Nachmansohn,  D.,  &  M.  A.  Rothenberg 

1945.  J.  Biol.  Chem.  158:  653. 

15.  Mendel,  B.,  &  H.  Rudney 
1945.     Science  100:  499. 


546  ANNALS  NEW  YORK  ACADEMY  OF  SCIENCES 

16.  Brown,  G.  L.,  &  W.  Feldberg 

1936.     J.  Physiol.  88:265. 

17.  Bernheim,  F.,  &  M.  L.  C.  Bernheim 

1936.  J.  Pharm.  Exp.  Therap.  57:  427. 

18.  Glick,  D. 

1937.  J.  Gen.  Physiol.  21:  289. 

19.  Nachmansohn,  D. 

1940.  Nature  145:513. 

20.  Massart,  L.,  &  R.  P.  DuFait 

1939.  Bull.  Soc.  Chim.  21:  1039. 

21.  Glick,  D. 

1941.  Nature  148:  662. 

22.  Straus,  O.  H.,  &  A.  Goldstein 
1943.     J.  Gen.  Physiol.  26:  559. 

23.  Easson,  L.  H.,  &  £.  Stedman 

1936.     Proc.  Roy.  Soc.  London  B  127:  142. 

24.  Collier,  H.  B.,  &  D.  E.  Allen 

1942.  Canad.  J.  Res.  20B:  189. 

25.  Ellis,  S.,  F.  L.  Plachte,  &  O.  H.  Straus 

1943.  J.  Pharm.  Exp.  Therap.  79:295. 

26.  Mazur,  A.,  &  O.  Bodansky 
1946.     J.  Biol.  Chem.  163:261. 

27.  Massart,  L.,  &  R.  P.  DuFait 

1940.  Enzymol.  9:364. 

28.  Glick,  D. 

1939.     J.  Pharm.  Exp.  Therap.  65:  389. 

29.  Zeller,  E.  A. 

1942.  Helvet.  Chim.  Acta  25:  216,  1099. 

30.  Nachmansohn,  D.,  &  H.  Schneeman 

1945.  J.  Biol.  Chem.  159:  239. 

31.  Torda,  C,  &  H.  G.  Wolff 

1944.  Proc.  Soc.  Exp.  Biol.  &  Med.  57:  236. 

32.  Heim,  F.,  &  W.  Rhode 

1943.  Arch.  Exp.  Path.  Pharm.  202:  216. 

33.  Matthes,  J. 

1930.     J.  Physiol.  70:  338. 

34.  Lange,  W.,  &  G.  v.  Krueger 

1932.  Ber.  65:  1598. 

35.  McCombie,  H.,  E.  D.  Adrian,  B.  A.  Kilby,  &  M.  Kilby 

1941.  Personal  communication. 

36.  Adrian,  E.  D.,  B.  A.  Kilby,  &  M.  Kilby 

1942.  Personal  communication. 

37.  Mackworth,  J.  F. 

1942.     Personal  communication. 

38.  Bodansky,  O. 

1933.  J.  Biol.  Chem.  101:  641. 

1936.  J.  Biol.  Chem.  115:  101. 

39.  Northrop,  J. 

1921.     J.  Gen.  Physiol.  2:  487. 

40.  Hall,  G.  E.,  &  G.  H.  Ettinger 

1937.  J.  Pharm.  Exp.  Therap.  59:  29. 

41.  Kerr,  W.  J.,  &  G.  H.  Whipple 
1919.     J.  Physiol.  47:356. 

42.  Leopold,  I.  H.,  &  J.  H.  Comroe 

1946.  Fed.  Proc.  6(11) :  190. 


BODANSKY:  CHOLIN ESTERASE  547 

43.  Comroe,  J.  H.,  J.  Todd,  G.  Gammon,  G.  B.  Koelle,  &  A.  Gilman 

1946.     Fed.  Proc.  5(11):  172. 

44.  Harvey,  A.  M.,  B.  F.  Jones,  S.  Talbot,  &  D.  Grob 
1946.     Fed.  Proc.  5(11) :  182. 

45.  Rothenberg,  M.  A.,  &  D.  Nachmansohn 
1946.     Fed.  Proc.  5(11) :  199. 

46.  Crescitelli,  F.,  G.  B.  Koelle,  &  A.  Gilman 
1946.     Fed.  Proc.  5(11):  172. 

47.  Easson,  L.  H.,  &  E.  Stedman 
1937.     Biochem.  J.  31:  1723. 

48.  Walker,  M.  B. 

1935.     Proc.  Roy.  Soc.  Med.  28:  759. 

49.  McGeorge,  M. 

1937.  Lancet  1:69. 

50.  Milhorat,  A.  T. 

1938.  J.  Clin.  Invest.  17:649. 

51.  Freudenberg,  R.,  &  F.  K.  Redlich 

1938.  Arch.  Exp.  Path.  Pharm.  188:  645. 

52.  Poncher,  H.  B.,  &  H.  W.  Wade 

1939.  Arch.  Neurol.  Psychiat.  41:  1127. 

53.  Stedman,  E.,  &  W.  R.  Russell 

1937.  Biochem.  J.  31:  1987. 

54.  Odom,  G.,  C.  K.  Russell,  &  D.  McEachern 
1943.     Brain  66:1. 

55.  Stoner,  H.  B.,  &  A.  Wilson 
1943.     J.  Physiol.  102:  1. 

56.  Jones,  W.  C,  &  W.  C.  Stadie 

1939.  Quart.  J.  Exp.  Physiol.  29:  63. 

57.  Gammon,  G.  D.,  A.  M.  Harvey,  &  R.  L.  Masland 
1941.     Biol.  Symposia  3:  291. 

58.  Glick,  D. 

1941.  Biol.  Symposia  5:  213. 

59.  Antopol,  W.,  A.  Schifrin,  &  L.  Tuchman 

1938.  Proc.  Soc.  Exp.  Biol.  &  Med.  38:  363. 

60.  McArdle,  B. 

1940.  Quart.  J.  Med.  9:  107. 

61.  Vahlquist,  B. 

1935.     Skand.  Arch.  Physiol.  72:  133. 

62.  Scoz,  G.,  &  C.  Cattaneo 
1937.     Enzymol.  4:  157. 

63.  Vincent,  D.,  &  J.  de  Prat 

1942.  C.  R.  Soc.  de  Biol.  136:821. 

64.  Faber,  M. 

1943.  Acta  Med.  Skand.  114:  59. 

65.  Antopol,  W.,  L.  Tuchman,  &  A.  Schifrin 

937.     Proc.  Soc.  Exp.  Biol.  &  Med.  36:  46. 

66.  Faber,  M. 

1943.     Acta  Med.  Skand.  114:  72. 

67.  Faber,  M. 

1943.     Acta  Med.  Skand.  115:  475. 

68.  Schutz,  F. 

1943.     Quart.  J.  Exp.  Med.  33 :  35. 

69.  Schutz,  F. 

1941.  Nature  148:  725. 

70.  Schutz,  F. 

1942.  jj^ J.  Physiol._102:  259,  269. 


THE  EFFECTS  OF  DRUGS  ON 
NERVE  ACTIVITY 

By  Alfred  Oilman* 

Pharmacology  Section,  Medical  Division,  Chemical  Warfare  Service,  Edgewood 

Arsenal,  Maryland 

I  cannot  help  but  feel  somewhat  apologetic  for  having  consented  to 
contribute  to  this  symposium.  Indeed,  as  the  date  for  this  conference 
approached,  it  became  more  and  more  apparent  that  I  had  become  in- 
volved in  an  extremely  paradoxical  situation.  On  first  thought,  it 
would  seem  reasonable  to  request  a  pharmacologist  to  discuss  the  sub- 
ject of  the  effects  of  drugs  on  nerve  activity.  However,  a  moment's 
reflection,  which  unfortunately,  from  my  point  of  view,  was  too  long 
delayed,  results  in  the  irrefutable  conclusion  that  it  is  the  neuro- 
physiologist  who  should  be  addressing  the  pharmacologists  on  this 
subject. 

Although  the  pharmacologist  has,  as  his  ultimate  objective,  the 
elucidation  of  the  fundamental  mechanism  of  action  of  drugs  on  cells, 
he  is  continually  frustrated  by  the  limitations  of  his  own  technics.  The 
very  nature  of  the  subject  of  pharmacology,  which  borders  on'  so  many 
medical  disciplines,  almost  precludes  the  possibility  of  the  investigator 
in  this  field  engaging  in  the  basic  research  which  is  essential  for  the 
reaching  of  his  objective,  except,  possibly,  in  a  chosen,  limited  field. 
It  is  from  the  neurophysiologist,  therefore,  that  the  answer  to  many  of 
the  basic  problems  of  the  actions  of  drugs  on  the  nervous  system  can 
be  expected. 

If  one  wishes  to  indulge  in  oversimplification,  the  entire  subject  of  the 
effects  of  drugs  on  nerve  activity  can  be  summarized  in  a  few  moments 
or  even  in  a  single  sentence.  There  is  no  phase  of  nerve  activity  which 
cannot  be  profoundly  affected  by  drugs.  Effector  cells  can  be  com- 
pletely released  from  nervous  control  or,  conversely,  the  effects  of  nerve 
impulses  can  be  faithfully  mimicked;  conduction  in  nerve  fibers  can  be 
completely  blocked;  synaptic  transmission  can  be  interrupted  or  en- 
hanced ;  cord  transection  can  be  simulated ;  selected  centers  in  the  brain 
can  be  stimulated  or  depressed.  It  only  remains  to  name  the  drugs 
associated  with  these  actions. 

Any  further  amplification  would  result  in  a  textbook  discussion, 
in  which,  in  a  more  or  less  orderly  fashion,  the  actions  of  drugs  could 

•  Major,  Sn-C,  A.  U.  S. 

(549) 


550  ANNALS  NEW  YORK  ACADEMY  OF  SCIENCES 

be  classified  in  such  general  terms  as  local  and  general  anesthetics, 
analgesics,  central  stimulants,  blocking  agents,  etc.  For  the  purpose 
of  understanding  the  therapeutic  application  of  drugs  modifying 
nervous  activity,  such  information  may  be  adequate.  However,  from 
the  standpoint  of  the  contributions  that  drugs  can  make  toward  the 
solution  of  basic  problems  in  neurophysiology,  our  understanding  of 
their  mechanism  of  action  is  as  yet  inadequate. 

Drugs  have  proved  to  be  invaluable  tools  in  many  fields  of  biological 
and  medical  research,  a  statement  which  the  neurophysiologist  would 
be  the  last  to  deny.  Their  value  is  readily  appreciated,  when  one  con- 
siders one  of  the  few  basic  and  general  statements  which  can  be  made 
concerning  the  fundamental  mechanism  of  action  of  drugs:  namely, 
that  drugs  cannot  impart  new  functions  to  cells  or  tissues,  but  can 
only  modify,  i.e.,  stimulate,  depress,  or  block,  functions  which  are  the 
fundamental  properties  of  that  cell  or  tissue.  Thus,  when  a  drug  pro- 
duces general  or  local  anesthesia,  has  a  convulsant  action,  blocks 
synaptic  transmission,  stimulates  chemoreceptors,  or  affects  nervous 
activity  in  any  of  a  variety  of  ways,  no  matter  how  extreme  the  re- 
sponse, the  assurance  is  justified,  until  an  exception  to  the  general  rule 
can  be  proved,  that  the  drug  in  question  has  modified  a  normal  cellular 
function. 

Examples  of  how  chemical  agents,  foreign  to  the  body,  have  con- 
tributed to  physiological  concepts  are  numerous.  Indeed,  through 
these  the  subject  matter  of  neurophysiology  has  been  enriched. 
Surely,  it  cannot  be  mere  coincidence  that  so  many  basic  contributions 
to  the  concept  of  the  chemical  transmission  of  the  nerve  impulse  had 
their  origin  in  pharmacological  laboratories?  Rather,  the  knowledge 
that  chemical  agents  could  mimic,  in  end-organs,  the  effect  of  nerve 
stimulation  served  as  the  basic  stimulus  for  the  search  for  evidence  of 
chemical  mediation.  Is  it  not  possible  that,  in  some  drug,  still  inade- 
quately explored  by  the  neurophysiologist,  lies  the  answer  to  a  basic 
neurophysiological  problem?  As  a  possible  example,  let  us  consider 
the  local  anesthetics.  The  local  anesthetic  action  of  cocaine  was  first 
demonstrated  in  1884.  This  type  of  action  has  been  shown  to  be 
exhibited  by  a  variety  of  chemical  structures,  but  the  tertiary  amino 
esters  of  benzoic  acid  and  para-amino  benzoic  acid,  as  a  group,  are 
capable  of  blocking  transmission.  These  compounds  show  no  respect 
for  any  classification  of  nerves,  but  block  cholinergic  and  adrenergic, 
sensory  and  motor  fibers  in  an  indiscriminate  manner,  which  points 
to  a  basic  action  on  a  fundamental  mechanism  of  transmission,  shared 
by  all  nerves.     Is  it  nnt  reasonable  to  suppose  that,  by  inquiring  more 


OILMAN:  EFFECTS  OF  DRV  OS  ON  NERVE  ACTIVITY         551 

deeply  into  the  action  of  drugs  such  as  the  local  anesthetics  on  a  cellular 
level,  information  may  be  gained  on  the  mechanism  of  the  propagation 
of  the  nerve  impulse? 

The  full  realization  of  the  contributions  that  drugs  can  make  to  the 
elucidation  of  fundamental  physiological  mechanisms,  can  only  result 
from  the  cooperative  research  efforts  of  the  pharmacologist  and  those 
investigators  who  are  focusing  their  interests  more  intensively  on  a 
specific  field.  This  is  readily  appreciated,  in  so  far  as  the  full  exploita- 
tion of  the  therapeutic  applications  of  a  drug  is  concerned.  The 
pharmacologist,  who,  in  the  course  of  an  investigation  on  central  de- 
pressants, finds  a  compound  with  new  and  significant  anti-convulsive 
properties,  will  invariably  refer  the  compound  to  a  clinical  neurologist, 
if  he  deems  it  worthy  of  consideration  as  an  anti-epileptic.  During 
the  course  of  chemical  warfare  research,  compounds  were  studied 
which,  to  the  pharmacologist,  suggested  themselves  as  potential  thera- 
peutic agents  in  the  treatment  of  such  unrelated  conditions  as  Hodg- 
kin's  disease,  mercury  poisoning,  Myasthenia  gravis,  and  glaucoma. 
In  every  instance,  the  prediction  of  therapeutic  worth  which  was  based 
upon  laboratory  analyses  proved  correct,  but  it  was  only  through  the 
efforts  of  clinicians,  highly  specialized  in  their  particular  field,  that  the 
full  appreciation  of  the  therapeutic  value  of  these  particular  agents 
was  realized. 

Drugs  are  constantly  following  the  path  from  laboratory  to  clinic, 
and  many  of  the  outstanding  medical  accomplishments  of  the  past 
decade  are  the  result  of  this  cooperative  effort.  However,  drugs  can 
only  make  their  full  contribution  to  the  science  of  medicine  when  they 
also  follow  another,  more  fundamental,  and  possibly  more  important 
path :  namely,  from  the  laboratory  of  the  pharmacologist  to  the  labora- 
tories of  investigators  working  on  those  physiological  problems  which 
attempt  to  define  biological  processes  in  their  most  fundamental  terms. 
That  drugs  are  not  being  fully  exploited,  in  this  respect,  is  a  regrettable 
fact.  Even  in  the  field  of  neurophysiology,  where  drugs  have  proveH 
such  valuable  research  tools,  a  full  realization  of  their  potential  contri- 
butions has  not  been  reached.  The  tendency,  rather,  has  been  to  accept 
drugs  with  known  actions  and  to  employ  them  for  these  actions,  rather 
than  to  investigate  unknown  mechanisms  of  drug  action  as  probes  into 
physiological  processes.  This  occurs  despite  the  fact  that  acetylcholine 
and  physostigmine,  two  drugs  which  are  better  understood  than  any 
other  compounds  affecting  nerve  action,  have  paved  the  way  toward 
revolutionary  concepts  in  an  understanding  of  synaptic  transmission. 

New  agents  affecting  the  nervous  system  are  constantly  being  de- 


552  ANNALS  NEW  YORK  ACADEMY  OF  SCIENCES 

scribed.  They  are  conveniently  labeled  as  convulsants,  depressants, 
etc.,  and  sporadic  attempts  are  made  to  localize  their  action,  following 
which,  investigation  is  considered  to  be  complete.  I  wish  to  take  this 
opportunity  to  present  three  new  compounds  which  have  come  to  the 
attention  of  those  who,  during  the  past  few  years,  have  been  workin:? 
in  the  field  of  chemical  warfare.  These  are  highly  toxic  agents  which 
must,  by  definition,  exert  profound  effects  on  basic  cellular  mechanisms. 
As  a  result  of  the  cooperative  research  program,  associated  with  the  war 
effort,  these  compounds  have  received  more  fundamental  study,  in  the 
course  of  a  few  years,  by  groups  with  more  divergent  interest,  than  is 
usually  the  case  with  better-known  and  more  widely-employed  drugs. 
These  three  compounds  have  been  selected  for  discussion  from  a  large 
group,  because  of  the  profound  actions  which  they  exert  on  the  nervous 
system. 

The  first  of  these  compounds  is  the  sodium  salt  of  fluoroacetic  acid. 
This  agent  has  had  an  interesting  history.  It  represents,  in  its  prac- 
tical applications,  one  of  the  many  fruitful  by-products  of  chemical 
warfare  research.  Sodium  fluoroacetate  was  screened  by  the  Fish  and 
Wildlife  Service  and  has  proved  to  be  by  far  the  most  outstanding  of 
all  rodenticides.  Now  known  as  Compound  1080,  it  promises  to  be 
an  important  agent  in  the  control  of  the  spread  of  disease  and  the  eco- 
nomic wastage  caused  by  rodents.  Yet  the  prediction  is  not  unjustified 
that  the  compound  may  prove  to  be  of  even  more  significant  value  as  a 
laboratory  research  tool,  assuming  equal  importance,  in  this  respect, 
with  the  iodoacetates. 

Although  the  fluoroacetate  ion  resembles  iodoacetate  in  structure,  it 
shares  none  of  its  chemical  or  physiological  properties.  However,  both 
halogenated  acetates  exert  profound  effects  on  cellular  metabolism. 
Highly  provocative  are  the  observations  of  Barron  and  co-workers,^ 
that  the  oxidation  of  acetate  by  yeast,  rat  kidney  suspensions,  and 
heart  slices  is  inhibited  by  fluoroacetate.  When  pyruvate  is  used  as 
the  oxidizable  substrate,  there  is  an  accumulation  of  acetate,  after  the 
addition  of  fluoroacetate,  and  the  synthesis  of  carbohydrate  from 
pyruvate  is  completely  inhibited.  Barron  has  advanced  the  reasonable 
hypothesis  that  fluoroacetate,  because  of  its  close  structural  relation- 
ship to  acetate,  blocks,  by  competitive  inhibition,  enzyme  systems  con- 
cerned with  the  utilization  of  acetate. 

When  one  considers  the  basic  importance  of  acetate  metabolism,  it 
is  of  extreme  interest  to  inquire  into  the  pharmacological  actions  of  a 
compound  which,  possibly,  interferes  with  the  utilization  of  this  essen- 
tial metabolite.     These  actions  have  been  investigated  by  Chenoweth 


OILMAN:  EFFECTS  OF  DRUGS  ON  NERVE  ACTIVITY         553 

and  Gilman.^  Species  vary  greatly,  both  in  their  response  and  toler- 
ance to  fluoroacetate.  In  general,  the  fluoroacetate  ion  possesses  two 
main  pharmacological  actions.  In  some  animals,  it  affects  primarily 
the  heart.  Energy  metabolism  and  conduction  are  so  disturbed  that 
pulsus  alternans,  A-V  block,  frequent  ectopic  ventricular  beats,  and 
eventually,  ventricular  fibrillation  result.  Most  herbivorous  animals, 
as  well  as  those  species  of  primates  that  have  been  studied,  are  sus- 
ceptible to  the  cardiac  actions  of  the  fluoroacetates.  The  actions  of  the 
fluoroacetates  on  the  nervous  system  are  even  more  striking.  Follow- 
ing a  latent  period  of  approximately  one  hour,  animals  become  progi'es- 
sively  more  excited,  and  eventually  exhibit  severe  epileptiform  con- 
vulsions which  continue,  uninterrupted,  until  death.  Dogs  and  cats 
respond  in  this  manner.  Most  of  those  species  which  exhibit  the  car- 
diac actions  show  no  evidence  of  central  stimulation.  However, 
primates,  although  succumbing  to  the  cardiac  action,  may  show  mild 
epileptiform  convulsions.  In  regard  to  susceptibility,  the  lethal  con- 
vulsive dose  in  the  dog  is  approximately  0.1  mg./Kg.  The  lethal  dose 
in  primates  is  approximately  100  times  as  great. 

When  one  considers  the  descriptive  pharmacology  of  fluoroacetate  in 
the  light  of  its  possible  fundamental  mechanism  of  action,  certain  ques- 
tions come  immediately  to  mind.  Are  differences  in  species  response 
due  to  different  metabolic  patterns  in  their  nervous  tissue?  Observa- 
tions of  Chenoweth  and  co-workers  support  the  view  that  the  primate 
myocardium  is  uniquely  dependent  upon  the  utilization  of  acetate,  for 
adequate  function.  Similar  investigations  may  reveal  that  the  same 
is  true  of  the  nervous  system  in  the  case  of  the  dog  and  cat.  It  has 
been  shown  by  Tepperman  and  Mazur^  that,  in  the  presence  of  fluoro- 
acetate, acetylation  is  greatly  enhanced,  presumably  because  of  the 
high  concentration  of  available  acetate.  Could  this  finding  possibly 
be  concerned  with  the  convulsive  action  of  the  fluoroacetate  ion? 
These  are  but  a  few  of  the  problems,  pertinent  to  the  nervous  system, 
that  have  been  raised  by  the  preliminary  investigations  of  this  drug. 
By  the  proper  utilization  of  fluoroacetate  as  a  research  tool,  it  may  be 
possible  to  relate  specific  disturbances  in  the  metabolism  of  nervous 
tissue  to  functional  abnormalities.  In  this  respect,  it  is  of  interest  to 
note  that  the  electroencephalogram  obtained  during  a  fluoroacetate- 
induced  convulsion  is  almost  identical  to  that  of  a  petit  mal  epileptic 
seizure. 

The  second  compound  to  be  discussed  is  diisopropyl-fluorophosphate. 
This  compound  represents  a  new  type  of  anticholinesterase.  Not  only 
does  it  depart,  in  its  chemical  configuration,  from  previously  studied 


554  ANNALS  NEW  YORK  ACADEMY. OF  SCIENCES 

anticholinesterase  agents,  but  it  also  differs  in  its  fundamental  mecha- 
nism of  action,  in  that  the  inhibition  of  cholinesterase  is  irreversible. 
What  is  more,  diisopropyl-fluorophosphate  is  highly  lipoid-soluble  and 
rapidly  gains  access  to  nervous  tissue. 

In  diisopropyl-jfiuorophosphate,  the  neurophysiologist  has  at  his 
command  a  research  tool  in  which  the  fundamental  mechanism  of  ac- 
tion is  known.  Thus,  if  the  major  premise  of  my  introductory  re- 
marks is  to  hold  true,  the  application  of  this  compound  to  problems  of 
neurophysiology  should  help  to  prove  or  disprove  fundamental  con- 
cepts of  nerve  function. 

The  advantages  of  an  irreversible  anticholinesterase,  as  a  research 
tool,  are  at  once  evident.  Following  the  action  of  diisopropyl-fluoro- 
phosphate,  the  cholinesterase  activity  of  a  tissue  can  be  restored  only 
by  resynthesis  of  enzyme.  Moreover,  the  agent  can  be  administered, 
and  the  response  of  a  tissue  studied.  Following  this,  the  tissue  can 
be  removed  and  ground ;  the  homogenate  appropriately  diluted ;  and  the 
absolute  cholinesterase  activity  determined ;  an  approach  which  cannot 
be  employed  with  a  compound  such  as  physostigmine,  due  to  the  re- 
versible nature  of  its  inhibition.  Thus,  for  the  first  time,  a  highly  quan- 
titative approach  to  the  problems  of  the  role  of  cholinesterase  and 
acetylcholine  in  the  transmission  of  the  nerve  impulse  is  available. 

Diisopropyl-fluorophosphate  has  already  followed  the  path  from 
the  laboratory  to  the  clinic.  Comroe  and  associates,^  at  the  University 
of  Pennsylvania,  and  Harvey  and  co-workers,^^  at  Johns  Hopkins,  have 
employed  this  agent  in  the  treatment  of  Myasthenia  gravis.  The 
therapeutic  efficacy  of  this  type  of  compound,  as  well  as  its  limitations, 
has  already  been  demonstrated.  Of  even  greater  interest,  will  be  the 
more  fundamental  data,  from  these  studies,  which  may  shed  light  on 
the  defect  in  transmission  associated  with  this  myopathy. 

Basic  laboratory  studies,  employing  diisopropyl-fluorophosphate  as 
a  research  tool,  have  also  begun.  I  should  like  to  report,  in  some  detail, 
the  experiments  of  Crescitelli  and  co-workers,®  designed  to  elucidate 
the  possible  role  of  acetylcholine  in  the  conduction  of  the  nerve  impulse 
along  the  nerve  fiber.  The  background  literature  to  this  problem  has 
recently  been  summarized  by  Loewi^  and  by  Feldberg,^  and  need  not 
be  repeated  here.  Mention  should  be  made,  however,  of  the  studies 
of  Cowan,^  Lorente  de  No,^"  Hertz,"  and  Cantoni  and  Loewi,^^  in 
which  either  physostigmine  or  acetylcholine  failed  to  exert  a  significant 
effect  on  transmission  in  the  nerve  fiber.  However,  the  availability 
of  an  irreversible  inhibitor  of  cholinesterase,  which  afforded  an  oppor- 
tunity to  correlate  nerve  function  with  quantitative  data  on  cholin- 


OILMAN:  EFFECTS  OF  DRUGS  ON  NERVE  ACTIVITY         555 

esterase  concentration,  prompted  a  repetition  of  this  type  of  study. 
Moreover,  advantage  was  taken  of  the  opportunity  to  compare  the 
effects  of  a  reversible  (physostigmine)  and  an  irreversible  anticholin- 
esterase. The  nerve  action  potential  was  employed  as  an  index  of 
effect  on  transmission.  It  was  argued  that,  whereas  both  types  of  anti- 
cholinesterase agent  should  affect  the  nerve  action  potential  in  the 
same  manner,  assuming  acetylcholine  to  play  a  major  role  in  trans- 
mission, the  action  of  physostigmine  should  prove  reversible,  that  of 
diisopropyl-fluorophosphate,  irreversible.  The  possibility  that  phy- 
sostigmine might  not  gain  access  to  those  structures  accessible  to  di- 
isopropyl-fluorophosphate was  avoided,  as  far  as  possible,  by  employ- 
ing the  alkaloid  base,  as  well  as  the  salicylate  salt. 

Two  types  of  experiments  were  performed.  In  the  first,  isolated 
nerves  of  bull  frogs  and  of  cats  were  mounted  in  a  moist  chamber, 
placed  in  a  constant  temperature  bath  of  appropriate  temperature.  A 
portion  of  the  nerve  was  looped  into  a  small  chamber  containing 
Ringer's  solution.  Following  the  recording  of  control  action  potentials, 
the  effects  of  the  various  drugs  were  ascertained.  When  the  isolated 
nerves  of  the  cat  or  the  bull  frog  were  exposed  to  0.01  molar  physostig- 
mine salicylate,  no  detectable  change  in  the  action  potential  was  ob- 
served. However,  when  the  solution  containing  the  salicylate  salt  of 
physostigmine  was  replaced  by  the  alkaloidal  base,  the  action  potential 
disappeared  within  a  period  of  10  minutes.  Washing  the  nerve  with 
Ringer's  solution  restored  the  action  potential  completely.  Thus,  the 
water-soluble  salicylate  salt  was  devoid  of  action,  whereas  the  lipoid- 
soluble  alkaloidal  base  blocked  transmission. 

Similar  experiments  were  then  performed,  by  exposing  the  nerve 
to  0.02  molar  diisopropyl-fluorophosphate.  Again,  the  action  poten- 
tial disappeared  within  a  few  minutes.  It  only  remained  to  demon- 
strate the  irreversibility  of  this  block,  in  order  to  attribute  the  effect 
to  the  inactivation  of  cholinesterase.  However,  washing  the  nerve 
restored  the  action  potential,  despite  the  fact  that  the  action  of  di- 
isopropyl-fluorophosphate was  supposedly  irreversible.  In  view  of 
this  surprising  result,  experiments  were  performed  to  determine  the 
extent  of  the  wash  necessary  to  restore  the  nerve  and  the  speed  at 
which  the  action  potential  returned.  During  the  course  of  these  studies, 
it  was  observed  that  it  was  only  necessary  to  remove  the  nerve  from 
contact  with  the  solution  containing  diisopropyl-fluorophosphate,  to 
restore  the  action  potential  completely.  Thus,  the  conduction  defect 
could  not  have  been  related  to  an  inhibition  of  cholinesterase. 

It  was  not  possible,  in  the  experiments  on  the  isolated  nerve,  to  meas- 
ure accurately  the  extent  of  inhibition  of  cholinesterase,  for  the  reason 


556  ANNALS  NEW  YORK  ACADEMY  OF  SCIENCES 

that  only  a  very  small  segment  of  nerve  was  exposed.  It  was,  thus, 
impossible  adequately  to  wash  the  nerve,  so  as  to  preclude  the  possi- 
bility of  mechanical  transfer  of  sufficient  diisopropyl-fluorophosphate 
to  inactivate  cholinesterase  during  the  preparation  of  the  nerve  for 
enzymatic  studies.  For  this  reason,  a  series  of  experiments  was  per- 
formed in  which  a  large  dose  of  diisopropyl-fluorophosphate  was  in 
jected  into  the  ventral  lymph  sac  of  frogs  and  allowed  to  reach  the 
nerve,  by  way  of  the  circulation.  After  a  suitable  interval,  the  nerves 
were  dissected  and  their  transmission  characteristics  studied  and  com- 
pared with  control  frogs.  Following  this,  the  cholinesterase  content  of 
the  control  and  experimental  nerves  was  studied.  Despite  the  fact 
that  the  nerves  of  the  experimental  frogs  were  completely  devoid  of 
cholinesterase,  the  transmission  of  the  nerve  impulse,  as  determined  by 
the  characteristics  of  the  action  potential,  in  response  to  single  and 
repetitive  stimuli,  was  unaffected.  This  finding  casts  serious  doubt 
on  the  role  of  acetylcholine  as  a  de-polarizer,  in  the  processes  of  con- 
duction along  the  axon. 

Loewi,  in  his  recent  review,  quotes  Dale  as  having  once  remarked 
that  it  was  unreasonable  to  suppose  that  nature  would  provide  for  the 
liberation  in  the  ganglion  of  acetylcholine,  the  most  powerful  stimulant 
of  ganglionic  cells,  for  the  sole  purpose  of  fooling  physiologists.  What, 
then,  is  the  function  of  cholinesterase  in  nerve  fibers,  which  Nachman- 
sohn  and  his  co-workers  have  shown  so  conclusively  to  be  concen- 
trated at  the  surface,  rather  than  in  the  axoplasm?  The  answer  is  not 
yet  forthcoming.  However,  in  a  drug  like  fluorophosphate,  it  is  possi- 
ble, by  localized  injection,  to  reduce  the  concentration  of  cholinesterase 
in  a  chosen  tissue  to  negligible  amounts.  Thus,  we  have  a  research  tool 
which  may  provide  the  answer  to  these  basic  problems. 

The  third  agent  will  be  discussed  only  very  briefly.  It  shares  with 
diisopropyl-fluorophosphate  the  ability  irreversibly  to  inactivate 
cholinesterase.  It  differs  from  diisopropyl-fluorophosphate  in  pos- 
sessing a  more  outstanding  action  on  the  nervous  system.  Certain  spe- 
cies, in  particular  cats  and  dogs,  exhibit  severe  convulsions,  which  have 
their  onset  within  a  few  minutes  after  the  intravenous  injection  of  the 
drug  and  which  persist  until  death.  The  fact  that  an  anticholinesterase 
agent  possesses  such  extreme  convulsant  action  could  possibly  be 
attributed  to  coincidence.  However,  there  is  one  finding  which  points 
to  an  intimate  relationship  between  convulsions  and  the  chemical 
mediation  of  central  synaptic  transmission.  If  the  animals  receive 
a  therapeutic  dose  of  atropine,  before  the  administration  of  this  anti- 
cholinesterase, no  convulsions  are  observed,  and  complete  protection 


OILMAN:  EFFECTS  OF  DRUOS  ON  NERVE  ACTIVITY         557 

is  afforded  from  what  would,  otherwise,  be  a  lethal  dose.  Moreover, 
if  the  agent  is  administered  and  the  convulsions  are  allowed  to  progress 
to  their  peak  intensity,  the  intravenous  injection  of  atropine  stops 
all  convulsive  activity  within  30  seconds,  and  the  animal  appears  nor- 
mal, as  soon  as  it  recovers  from  its  exhaustion.  It  should  be  empha- 
sized that  the  doses  of  atropine  that  exert  this  anticon\^lsant  action 
are  of  a  small  magnitude  and,  in  themselves,  exert  no  demonstrable 
central  effects.  No  other  central  stimulant  can  be  inhibited  in  this 
manner.  The  effect  of  atropine,  in  blocking  the  convulsant  action  of 
this  anticholinesterase,  is  as  dramatic  and  as  complete  as  is  the  effect  of 
atropine  in  blocking  the  reception  of  post-ganglionic  cholinergic  im- 
pulses by  autonomic  effector  cells.  It  seems  certain  that,  in  this  com- 
pound, there  is  a  research  tool  which  can  make  a  significant  contribu- 
tion to  the  fundamental  problems  of  central  synaptic  transmission. 

During  the  past  few  years,  the  group  of  investigators  at  Edgewood 
Arsenal  has  been  engaged  in  a  cooperative  research  effort,  in  which  toxic 
war  gases  or,  in  other  words,  highly  active  drugs,  were  the  focal  point  of 
their  investigations.  Their  efforts  were  coordinated  with  extensive 
programs  of  numerous  academic  groups.  From  the  point  of  view  of 
the  pharmacologist,  this  elaborate  approach  to  the  mechanism  of  drug 
action  has  proved  to  be  an  illuminating  experience.  One  cannot  fail  to 
be  impressed  by  the  fact  that,  as  the  story  of  each  agent  unfolded,  its 
potential  value  toward  the  solution  of  fundamental  problems  in  phy- 
siology and  biochemistry  was  more  and  more  appreciated. 

I  have  departed  from  a  routine  discussion  of  the  effects  of  drugs  on 
nervous  activity,  to  present  to  you  three  new  agents  which  have  re- 
sulted from  this  program.  It  may  be  predicted  that,  as  research  tools, 
they  will  prove  invaluable.  If  so,  then  the  contention  made  earlier  will 
have  been  fulfilled:  that,  by  tracing  the  actions  of  drugs  to  their  cellular 
mechanisms,  basic  physiological  processes  will  be  revealed. 

REFERENCES 

1.  Barron,  E.  S,  G.,  G.  R.  Bartlett,  &  G.  Kalnitsky 
1946.     Fed.  Proc.  5(11):  120. 

2.  Chenoweth,  M.  B.,  &  A.  Gilman 
1946.     J.  Pharm.  Exp.  Therap.     87;  90. 

3.  Tepperman,  J.,  &  A.  Mazur 
Per.sonal  Comiiunication. 

4.  Comroe,  J.  H.,  Jr.,  J.  Todd,  G.  Gammon,  G.  B.  Koelle,  &  A.  Gilman 
1946.     Fed.  Proc.  5(11) :  172. 

5.  Harvey,  A.  McG.,  B,  F.  Jones,  S.  Talbot,  &  D.  Grob 

1946.     Fed.  Proc.  5(11):  182. 

6.  Crescitelli,  F.  N.,  G.  B.  Koelle,  &  A.  Gilman 

1946.     J.  Neurophysiol.     9:24. 


558  ANNALS  NEW  YORK  ACADEMY  OF  SCIENCES 

7.  Loewi,  O. 

1945.     J.  Mount  Sinai  Hosp.  12:  851. 

8.  Feldberg,  W. 

1945.     Physiol.  Rev.  25:  596. 

9.  Cowan,  S.  L. 

1938.     J.  Physiol.  93:215. 

10.  Lorente  de  No,  R. 

1944.  J.  Cell.  Comp.  Physiol.  24:  86. 

11.  Hertz,  H. 

1945.  J.  Physiol.  104:  1. 

12.  Cantoni,  G.  L.,  &  O.  Loewi 

1944.     J.  Pharm.  Exp.  Therap.  81:  67. 


THE    RECOVERY    OF    DIAMETER   AND    IMPULSE 

CONDUCTION  IN  REGENERATING 

NERVE  FIBERS 

By  Charles  M.  Berry  and  Joseph  C.  Hinsey 

Department  oj  Ayiatomy,  Cornell  University  Medical  College,  New  York,  N.  Y. 

The  primary  purpose  of  these  experiments  was  to  study  those  prop- 
erties of  regenerating  fibers  which  could  be  observed  oscillographically 
and  related  to  histological  controls.  Therefore,  the  contours  of  the 
action  potentials,  the  conduction  velocities  of  the  impulses,  and  fiber 
diameters  were  followed  in  a  series  of  cat  nerves.  The  recovery  of 
these  properties  might  be  considered  to  be  a  process  of  reconstitution  or 
maturation,  as  opposed  to  the  longitudinal  outgrowth  of  the  fibers,  and 
since  these  properties  continued  to  change  over  long  periods  of  regenera- 
tion, measured  in  years,  the  experiments  were  spread  over  a  wide  range 
of  time,  from  a  few  days  to  more  than  three  years. 

Having  observed  the  effects  of  time  (which  is  undoubtedly  the  most 
important  factor  in  the  reconstitution  of  fibers  distal  to  a  suture) ,  we 
extended  the  experiments  to  include  the  effects  of  crushing  the  nerves 
compared  to  section  and  suture;  the  effects  of  delaying  the  suture  after 
transection;  and  the  effects  of  cross-suturing  nerves  containing  fibers 
of  different  fiber  diameters.  The  importance  of  these  factors  has  been 
reemphasized  in  recent  publications.  The  growth  of  fiber  diameters 
in  the  distal  stump  over  a  one  year  period  has  been  carefully  plotted 
by  Gutmann  and  Sanders.^  Furthermore,  they  showed  differences  in 
recovery  between  crushed  and  sutured  nerves.  The  influences  of  phy- 
sical stresses  in  the  '''union"  tissue  described  by  Weiss-  show  the  im- 
portance of  the  type  of  junction  between  central  and  distal  stumps. 
The  effects  of  delaying  the  suture  after  section  of  a  peripheral  nerve 
were  studied  by  Holmes  and  Young,^  and  the  effects  of  cross-suturing 
visceral  and  somatic  nerves  were  reported  by  Simpson  and  Young.* 
Young  and  his  co-workers  have  paid  special  attention  to  the  connective 
tissue  sheath  diameters  in  the  distal  stump. 

METHODS  AND  RESULTS 

Action  Potentials  from  Regenerating  Nerves 
Cathode-ray    oscillographs    were    taken    from    regenerating   tibial, 
peroneal,  and  saphenous  nerves  of  64  cats.     The  nerves  were  transected 

(559) 


560  ANNALS  NEW  YORK  ACADEMY  OF  SCIENCES 

with  a  sharp  blade  and  immediately  sutured  with  silk.  Each  nerve 
was  allowed  to  regenerate  for  a  determined  interval,  from  17  to  1363 
days,  and  was  then  excised  and  placed  on  electrodes  in  a  moist  chamber 
at  38°  C. 

The  regenerating  fibers  were  able  to  conduct  impulses  after  a  very 
brief  regeneration  period,  and  at  17  days,  action  potentials  were  re- 
corded from  the  distal  stump  of  one  tibial  nerve  within  a  distance  of 
2  cm.  from  the  suture.  This  potential  was  small,  less  than  10  micro- 
volts in  amplitude,  and  was  conducted  very  slowly  at  a  maximum  of 
0.9  meters  per  second.  The  potentials  recorded  from  fibers  which  had 
regenerated  for  longer  periods  were  of  greater  amplitude  and  were  con- 
ducted at  greater  velocities.  The  increase  in  conduction  velocity  was 
rapid  in  the  first  few  days  and,  by  36  days,  velocities  of  17  m.p.s.  were 
recorded,  as  shown  in  plate  5  A.  This  record  was  taken  from  a  mono- 
polar electrode  placed  3.5  cm.  distal  to  the  suture,  and  shows  a  maxi- 
mum conduction  velocity  of  17  m.p.s.  and  a  spike  amplitude  of  25 
microvolts.  The  maximum  conduction  velocity  continued  to  increase 
with  the  time  allowed  for  regeneration,  but  at  an  ever-decreasing  rate 
of  recovery.  Thus,  at  50  days,  25  m.p.s.  were  attained;  at  100  days, 
40  m.p.s.;  at  200  days,  60  m.p.s.;  at  365  days,  70  m.p.s.  Beyond  544 
days,  no  further  recovery  of  conduction  velocity  was  found,  and  at  the 
long  period  of  1363  days,  only  80  m.p.s.  were  attained.  The  record  in 
PLATE  5  C  was  taken  from  the  distal  stump  of  a  tibial  nerve  1363  days 
after  suture,  and  can  be  compared  with  the  record  from  the  opposite, 
normal,  tibial  nerve  of  the  same  animal  in  plate  5  B,  in  order  to  deter- 
mine the  degree  of  recovery.  In  plate  5,  B  and  C,  the  conduction  dis- 
tance was  8  cm.,  but  in  the  record  from  the  regenerated  nerve,  the  dis- 
tance or  time  between  the  shock  artifact  and  the  beginning  of  the  spike 
is  greater  than  in  the  normal  record,  and  shows  that  the  80  m.p.s.  repre- 
sent less  than  80%  recovery  toward  the  normal  conduction  velocity. 
Similar  results  were  obtained  from  the  peroneal  and  saphenous  nei'ves. 

Two  other  observations  can  be  made  from  plate  5,  B  and  C.  Firstly, 
the  amplitude  of  the  spike  is  less  in  the  regenerated  nerve,  and  secondly, 
the  spike  in  plate  5  C  is  not  as  complex.  The  lack  of  recovery  of  all 
the  components  of  the  spike  was  even  more  obvious  in  records  from 
the  saphenous  nerves,  where  the  normal  potential  is  more  complex  and 
consists  of  a  double  or  triple  peaked  alpha  wave  and  distinct  beta, 
gamma,  and  delta  waves.  Even  after  long  periods  of  regeneration,  the 
saphenous  nerve  did  not  recover  these  wave  components  and  showed 
only  an  initial  peak  which  leveled  off  into  a  long  tail. 


BERRY— HINSEY:  RECOVERY,  REGENERATING  NERVES      551 

An  accurate  method  of  determining  the  conduction  velocities  of  the 
most  rapidly  conducting  fibers  is  illustrated  in  figure  1.  Several 
records  were  taken  along  a  regenerating  nerve  at  various  conduction 
distances,  and  either  the  stimulating  or  the  recording  electrodes  were 
placed  at  a  fixed  point  along  the  nerve.  Then  the  other  electrodes  were 
moved  stepwise,  to  provide  a  greater  conduction  distance  for  each  suc- 
cessive record.  Thus,  in  figure  1,  the  stimulating  electrodes  were 
placed  4.6  cm.  distal  to  the  suture,  and  the  pair  of  recording  electrodes 
was  placed  at  a  variety  of  points  both  distal  and  proximal  to  the 


lOr- 


i 


0 
c 
o 

in 

•0 

c 

o 

0 

;3 
c 

o 

o 


8- 


- 

/ 

- 

•  ^^ 

1 

1     1     1     1     1     1     1     1 

1    1    1    1    1    1 

2- 


0.5  10 

Conduction  time  -  msec. 


1.5 


"'^  Central 
—  8       end 

-7 

-6 

*-5 

) Suture 

-3 

■^^  Distal 
<-jL      end 


Figure  1.  Graph  of  conduction  distance  against  conduction  time  of  the  action  potentials,  from 
a  saphenous  nerve,  58  days  after  suture.  The  diagram  on  the  right  sliows  that  the  stimulating 
electrodes  were  held  stationary  on  the  distal  stump,  while  the  recording  electrodes  were  moved. 
The  conduction  velocity  jumped  from  23  m./s.  in  the  distal  stump  to  50  m./s.  in  the  central  stump. 
(Berry,  Grundfest,  &  Hinsey.') 

suture.  At  each  distance,  a  record  was  taken,  and  the  conduction  time 
was  measured  between  the  shock  and  the  initial  rise  of  the  spike.  When 
this  time  was  plotted  against  conduction  distance,  as  in  figure  1,  the 
slope  of  the  line  indicated  the  maximum  conduction  velocity. 

The  change  in  the  slope  of  the  line,  in  figure  1,  occurred  at  the  suture 
line  which  shows  that  the  distal  outgrowths  conduct  more  slowly  than 
their  parent  fibers  in  the  central  stump.  The  actual  velocities  from  this 
saphenous  nerve,  58  days  after  suture,  were  23  m.p.s.  distal  to  the 
suture,  and  these  same  fibers  central  to  the  suture  conducted  at  50 
m.p.s.  The  continuity  of  the  plotted  line  and  its  straight  contour  cen- 
tral to  the  suture  demonstrates  that  the  change  in  velocity  was  recorded 
from  identical  fibers  on  both  sides  of  the  suture. 

Assuming  that  conduction  velocity  is  related  to  the  fiber  diameter, 
the  electrical  method  proves  that  the  small  fibers  of  the  distal  stump 
are  not  the  result  of  a  selective  ability  of  only  the  small  fibers  of  the 


562  ANNALS  NEW  YORK  ACADEMY  OF  SCIENCES 

central  stump  to  grow  out  into  the  distal  stump,  before  the  larger  fibers 
can  grow.  Conversely,  it  demonstrates  that  large  fibers  of  the  central 
stump  send  out  small  extensions  into  the  distal  stump,  which  then  ma- 
ture and  take  on  the  action  potential  characteristic  of  smaller  fibers, 
regardless  of  their  origin. 

A  consistent  finding,  not  illustrated  in  figure  1,  was  that  the  conduc- 
tion velocity  of  the  distal  fibers  was  less  at  greater  distances  from  the 
suture. 

Fiber  Diameter  Measurements 

The  regenerating  tibial,  peroneal,  and  saphenous  nerves  which  had 
been  excised  and  used  for  action  potential  experiments  were  fixed  in 
osmic  acid  and  studied  microscopically.  The  most  obvious  change  in 
the  character  of  the  regenerating  nerves,  as  they  were  allowed  to  grow 
for  longer  and  longer  periods,  was  the  gradual  increase  in  fiber  diam- 
eters. The  outside  diameters,  including  both  axis  cylinders  and  myelin 
sheaths,  were  measured  with  a  movable,  ocular  micrometer.  To  insure 
random  sampling,  the  fibers  were  measured  in  horizontal  bands,  with 
approximately  500  fibers  measured  in  each  nerve.  The  results  were 
plotted  along  fiber  distribution  curves,  as  illustrated  in  figure  2.  These 
nine  histograms  were  picked  from  a  series  of  regenerating  tibial  nerves, 
to  show  the  diameter  characteristics  of  the  fibers  in  the  distal  segments 
at  different  time  intervals  following  suture.  Since  the  diameters  were 
smaller,  at  greater  distances  from  the  suture,  the  portions  of  tibial 
nerves  studied  in  each  of  the  histograms  in  figure  2  were  taken  from 
similar  levels,  just  beyond  the  upper  branches  to  the  gastrocnemius 
muscles,  3  to  5  cm.  distal  to  the  sutures. 

The  shift  in  distribution  from  left  to  right  in  the  histograms  of 
FIGURE  2  shows  rapid  diameter  growth  between  33,  59,  and  127  days 
after  transection  and  suture.  At  longer  regeneration  times  of  207,  318, 
and  420  days,  the  fibers  continued  to  mature,  but  more  slowly.  At 
544,  901,  and  1363  post-operative  days,  there  was  negligible  increase 
in  fiber  diameter,  but  a  complete  recovery  of  the  normal  fiber  size  was 
never  attained.  Even  at  1363  post-operative  days,  the  large  group  of 
fibers  between  9  and  20  micra  had  not  appeared.  Similar  lack  of  com- 
plete recovery  of  fiber  size  was  found  in  the  peroneal  and  saphenous 
nerves. 

Particular  attention  was  paid  to  the  measurement  of  the  largest 
fibers  in  each  nerve,  since  the  maximum  conduction  velocity  had  al- 
ready been  determined  accurately,  and  it  was  a  reasonable  assumption 
that  the  largest  fibers  were  responsible  for  the  maximum  conduction 
velocity.     The  growth  of  the  largest  fibers  is  illustrated  in  figure  3 


BERRY— HINSEY:  RECOVERY,  REGENERATING  NERVES      563 


40- 

»-   30- 

Z 

UJ 

33 
DAYS 

O   20- 

UJ 

Q-     10- 

-| 

2 

-i:)- 

UJ 

CD    20 


FIBER    DIAMETER    IN    MICRA 

Figure  2.  Reconstitution  of  fiber  diameter  with  regeneration  time.  Each  fiber  diameter  distri- 
bution graph  is  from  a  different  nerve,  excised  on  the  indicated  number  of  days  after  transection 
and  suture. 


(6-1 


14- 


<    \2-\ 

a: 

o 


q: 


UJ 

< 


6- 


tr    4. 

bJ 

m 
u. 
2 


-r 


T 


300  600  900 

REGENERATION    TIME    IN    DAYS 


1200 


FiCTRB  3.    Outside  diameters  of  the  largest  fibers  in  the  distal  stumps  of  sutured  tibial  nerves. 
The  fibers  do  not  attain  the  normal  maximum  diameters,  above  20  micra. 


564 


ANNALS  NEW  YORK  ACADEMY  OF  SCIENCES 


as  a  graph  of  fiber  diameter  against  the  amount  of  time  allowed  for  re- 
generation after  suture  of  the  tibial  nerves.  The  leveling  off  of  the 
diameter  growth  curve  at  a  level  between  14  and  16  micra  again  illus- 
trates the  incomplete  "maturation"  of  fibers,  even  after  long  periods 
of  regeneration. 

The  Relationship  between  the  Conduction  Velocity  and  the 

Fiber  Diameter 

The  results  from  both  the  action  potential  and  the  fiber  diameter 
studies  showed  a  gradual  recovery,  which  tapered  off  with  no  complete 

100 


"--Tibial 
•  ^Peroneal 
A = Saphenous 


6  8  10  12 

Tibep  diametep  -  /j. 

Figure  4.  Graph  showing  the  relationship  between  the  maximum  conduction  velocities  and  the 
outside  diameters  of  regenerating  fibers  of  the  nerves:  tibial  (crosses),  peroneal  (circles),  and 
saphenous  (triangles).    (Berry,  Grundfest,  &  HinseyJ) 

return  of  either  the  velocity  of  impulse  conduction  or  the  size  of  the 
fibers.  The  actual  relationship  between  the  two  functions  is  shown  in 
FIGURE  4,  where  the  maximum  conduction  velocity  is  plotted  against 


BERRY— HINSEY:  RECOVERY,  REGENERATING  NERVES      565 

the  maximum  fiber  diameter  for  each  nerve.  The  linear  relationship 
between  the  two  functions  is  thus  illustrated  in  a  new  way,  and  con- 
firms the  contention  of  Gasser  and  Grundfest,^  that  the  relationship  is 
a  linear  one.  Less  deviation  from  the  straight  line  in  figure  4  was 
found  at  the  lower  end  of  the  graph,  if  the  inside  diameter  (without 
myelin  sheath)  was  measured  instead  of  outside  diameter. 

The  Difference  in  Recovery  between  Sutured  and  Crushed  Nerves 

In  a  small  series  of  10  cats,  the  tibial,  peroneal,  and  saphenous  nerves 
were  crushed  with  thin,  flat-surfaced  forceps,  and  the  nerves  were  al- 
lowed to  regenerate  for  determined  intervals.  The  purpose  of  these 
experiments  was  to  determine  whether  the  recovery  of  the  action  poten- 
tial and  fiber  diameter  would  occur  in  the  same  way  as  had  been  ob- 
served in  the  sutured  nerves. 


25 


UJ 

o 

UJ 

Q. 


CO 
QC 
UJ 
CO 


o 

cr 

UJ 
CD 


20 


15- 


10 


5- 


"1 — T — I — i — I — I — I — 1~      I — I — I — I      I      I      I      1      I      r 
23456789  123456789 

FIBER    DIAMETER    IN    MICRA 


10 


FiGUBE  5.    Comparison  of  fiber  distribution  according  to  diameter. 
Histogram  on  the  left,  after  suture. 
Histogram  on  the  right,  after  crush. 
Both  tibial  nerves  were  allowed  to  regenerate  for  94  days. 

The  action  potential  records  from  the  crushed  nerves  showed  greater 
recovery  of  conduction  velocity  and  magnitude  of  the  spikes  than  was 
found  in  records  from  comparable  regions  of  sutured  nerves,  taken 
after  the  same  amount  of  time  had  been  allowed  for  regeneration.  The 
fibers  also  grew  in  diameter  more  rapidly  in  the  crushed  nerves.  In 
FIGURE  5,  the  histogram  on  the  left  is  from  the  distal  stump  of  the  tibial 
nerve,  94  days  after  suturing,  that  on  the  right,  from  a  tibial  nerve,  94 
days  after  crushing.     Shift  of  the  graph  to  the  right,  in  the  c 


566  ANNALS  NEW  YORK  ACADEMY  OF  SCIENCES 

nerve  compared  to  the  sutured,  demonstrates  the  more  rapid  recovery 
of  fiber  size  in  the  crushed  nerves.  These  experiments  were  not  carried 
beyond  200  days,  and  the  ultimate  amount  of  recovery  of  the  normal 
fiber  distributions  was  not  determined  for  crushed  nerves. 

The  Effects  of  Delayed  Suture  on  Fiber  Diameter  Growth 

The  method  of  studying  the  "maturation"  of  the  fibers  by  action 
potential  records  and  diameter  measurements  was  used  to  determine 
the  influence  of  delaying  the  suture  after  transection.  An  operative 
procedure  similar  to  that  of  Holmes  and  Young^  was  devised,  to  allow 
the  tibial  nerve  to  degenerate  after  section  and  remain  free  of  fibers 
during  a  determined  delay  period.  The  adjacent  peroneal  nerve  was 
then  transected,  and  the  freshly  cut  central  stump  was  sutured  to  the 
old  distal  remnant  of  the  tibial.  This  cross-suture  was  used  to  limit 
the  study  to  effects  of  delay  in  the  distal  segment. 

In  the  same  animal,  a  reliable  control  was  provided  in  each  experi- 
ment by  suturing  the  peroneal  nerve  of  the  opposite  leg  to  the  tibial 
nerve.  This  was  done  in  exactly  the  same  manner  as  on  the  delayed 
side,  but,  in  this  case,  there  was  no  delay  between  section  and  suture. 

The  procedure  may  be  summarized  as  follows:  The  right  tibial  nerve 
was  exposed,  and  a  long  segment  removed  from  the  sciatic  notch  to  the 
popliteal  space.  To  insure  the  absence  of  regeneration  during  the  de- 
lay period,  the  cut  nerve  was  exposed  at  6-month  intervals.  Then,  after 
a  delay  of  14  to  476  days,  a  second  operation  was  performed,  in  which 
the  right  peroneal  was  sectioned  and  the  central  end  sutured  in  the  old 
distal  tibial.  At  this  same  time,  the  left  peroneal  was  sectioned  and 
sutured  to  the  distal  tibial  as  a  control.  105  to  440  days  were  then 
allowed  for  regeneration  before  the  terminal  experiment. 

The  shapes  of  the  action  potentials  and  the  maximum  conduction 
velocities  in  the  distal  segments,  after  delay  periods  of  14,  21,  28,  56, 
84,  and  180  days,  were  similar  to  those  recorded  from  the  nerves  of  the 
opposite  leg  which  had  been  sutured  without  delay.  In  each  of  these 
experiments,  105  days  were  allowed  for  regeneration  after  suture.  The 
diameter  distribution  of  the  fibers  was  the  same  on  both  sides,  as  illus- 
trated in  FIGURE  6,  A  and  B.  The  histogram  in  figure  6  B  was  from 
the  right  tibial,  3  cm.  distal  to  the  suture,  which  was  delayed  84  days, 
and  after  which  the  nerve  regenerated  for  105  days  before  the  fibers 
were  measured.  The  control  from  the  same  animal  is  shown  in  figure 
6  A.  Both  nerves  were  analyzed  105  days  after  suture.  The  similarity 
of  the  two  histograms  was  also  found  with  14,  21,  28,  56,  and  180  days, 
and  indicates  that  such  delay  periods  had  no  influence  on  the  diameter 
growth  of  fibers,  under  these  experimental  conditions. 


BERRY— HINSEY:  RECOVERY,  REGENERATING  NERVES      567 


A  suture  delayed  for  a  longer  period  of  253  days  resulted  in  less  re- 
constitution  of  the  fiber  diameters  in  the  peripheral  stump  than  that 
found  in  the  control.  The  histogram  in  figure  6  D  was  from  the  dis- 
tal tibial,  3  cm.  distal  to  the  suture,  which  was  delayed  253  days. 


DIAMETER    IN    MICRA 


Figure  6.  The  effects  of  delayed  suture  on  fiber  diameter  growth  in  the  distal  stumps  of  re- 
generating nerves.  Control  sutures  with  no  delay,  on  the  left  (A,  C,  E) ;  delayed  sutures,  on  the 
right  (B,  D,  F). 

A  is  from  th^  left  tibial  nerve,  105  days  after  immediate  suture;  B  is  from  the  right  nerve  of 
the  same  cat,  105  days  after  a  suture  which  was  delayed  84  days;  C  and  D,  440  days  after  suture 
with  253  days  delay  in  D ;   E  and  F,  337  days  after  suture  with  476  days  delay  in  F. 

Figure  6  C  was  from  the  same  level  of  the  opposite  tibial,  with  no  delay. 
Both  nerves  were  allowed  to  regenerate  for  440  days  after  suture,  and 
it  must  be  pointed  out  that  the  longer  regeneration  period  may  be  an 
important  factor.  The  fibers  in  figure  6  D  were  smaller  throughout, 
with  a  maximum  diameter  under  11  micra.  On  the  control  side,  figure 
6  C,  the  fibers  were  generally  larger,  with  a  few  reaching  14  micra. 

The  effects  of  delay  were  even  more  marked  after  476  days,  as  shown 
in  FIGURE  6,  E  and  F.  The  histogram  in  F,  after  476  days  delay,  shows 
most  of  the  fibers  to  be  less  than  4  or  5  micra,  while  the  histogram  of 


568  ANNALS  NEW  YORK  ACADEMY  OF  SCIENCES 

the  control  in  E  shows  much  larger  fibers  throughout  (figure  6) .     Both 
nerves  were  allowed  to  regenerate  337  days  after  suture. 

The  maximum  delay  which  caused  no  change  in  fiber  growth  could 
not  be  determined  accurately  from  these  experiments,  because  the 
regeneration  time  was  not  kept  constant.  However,  in  those  experi- 
ments with  253  and  440  days  delay,  approximately  a  year  was  allowed 
for  regeneration.  These  experiments  were  more  conclusive,  and  indi- 
cated that  such  delay  periods  restrict  the  diameter  growth  of  the  re- 
generating fibers.  Not  only  were  the  largest  caliber  fibers  limited  in 
growth,  but  the  whole  fiber  distribution  curve  was  altered. 

The  Effects  of  Cross- Suturing  Nerves  of  Different  Fiber  Caliber 

On  the  basis  of  cross-suture  experiments,  Simpson  and  Young'*  de- 
scribed a  restrictive  influence  on  fiber  diameter  growth  by  very  small 
Schwann  tubes.  Using  a  similar  approach,  Hammond  and  Hinsey® 
cross-sutured  the  hypoglossal  nerve  and  the  cervical  sympathetic 
trunk.  The  choice  of  these  nerves  was  fortunate,  since  they  contain 
much  different  tube  diameter  distributions,  and  they  are  situated  close 
^  together  for  easy  manipulation  for  cross-suturing. 

Fiber  diameter  distributions  of  the  normal  hypoglossal  nerves,  meas- 
ured from  osmic  preparations,  showed  large  fibers,  between  2.3  and  17 
micra  in  outside  diameter,  with  a  unimodal  peak  between  6.5  and  8.5 
micra.  Calculation  from  11  hypoglossal  nerves  showed  the  median 
fiber  diameter  to  be  7.7  micra,  with  only  3%  of  the  fibers  smaller  than 
4.5  micra.  Similar  observation  on  the  cervical  sympathetic  trunk  at 
its  rostral  end  showed  relatively  small  fibers,  between  1.2  and  8.4 
micra  in  diameter,  with  a  unimodal  peak  at  2.5  to  3.5  micra.  The 
median  diameter  calculated  from  7  experiments  was  3.3  micra,  and 
only  7%  of  the  fibers  were  larger  than  4.5  micra. 

In  order  to  compare  the  effects  of  suture  of  a  nerve  with  large  fibers 
into  a  nerve  with  smaller  fibers  and  the  effects  of  a  control  experiment 
in  which  the  nerve  with  large  fibers  was  sutured  into  its  own  distal 
stump,  it  was  necessary  to  run  simultaneous  experiments,  with  the 
nerves  excised  and  measured  at  the  same  intervals  after  suture.  There- 
fore, in  one  set  of  experiments,  the  hypoglossal  nerves  were  sectioned 
and  immediately  joined  to  their  own  distal  stumps.  The  fiber  distribu- 
tion in  the  distal  stumps  of  these  hypoglossal-to-hypoglossal  sutures 
was  determined  as  illustrated  in  the  top  row  of  histograms  in  figure  7. 
The  progress  of  diameter  growth  of  the  fibers  is  indicated  by  a  shift 
of  the  curves  from  left  to  right,  as  the  time  of  regeneration  increased 
to  216,  250,  300,  and    365  days.     This  increase  is  similar  to  that  found 


BERRY— HINSEY:  RECOVERY,  REGENERATING  NERVES      569 


in  the  experiments  on  the  tibial,  peroneal,  and  saphenous  nerves,  and 
about  80%  recovery  of  normal  fiber  diameter  was  found  at  365  days. 


.5      3.5     6.5     9.5  .5 

FIBER    DIAMETER 


I  I  I  I  I  I  I  I  I  I  I  I 

,5       3.5     6.5     9.5 


IN    MICRA 


Figure  7.  The  top  row  of  fiber  distribution  graphs  is  from  dista!  stumps,  after  simple  suture  of 
the  hypoglossal  nei-\-e;  bottom  row,  from  the  distal  stump,  when  the  central  end  of  the  hypo- 
glossal was  sutured  to  the  cervical  .sympathetic  trunk. 

The  time  allowed  for  regeneration  in  both  the  top  and  bottom  rows  is  indicated  in  days. 
(Modified  from  Hammond  &  Hinsey.") 

With  the  diameter  studies  of  the  hypoglossal-to-hypoglossal  series 
as  a  basis  for  comparison,  the  suture  of  the  hypoglossal  nerve  to  the 
cervical  sympathetic  trunk  produced  remarkably  different  results. 
The  lower  row  of  histograms  in  figure  7  shows  the  fiber  distributions  in 
the  distal  stumps  (cervical  sympathetic),  at  the  same  intervals  of 
regeneration  as  those  in  the  upper  row  of  hypoglossal-to-hypoglossal 
experiments.  At  216  days,  the  histogram  of  the  distal  segment  shown 
in  the  lower  row  was  not  much  different  than  that  of  the  distal  segment 
in  the  upper  row.  However,  at  250,  300,  and  365  days,  the  fibers  did 
not  continue  to  grow  as  in  the  simple  hypoglossal-to-hypoglossal  suture, 
but,  instead,  the  diameters  decreased.  By  365  days,  the  distal  segment 
of  the  hypoglossal-to-cervical  sympathetic  cross-suture  took  on  the 
diameter  characteristics  of  the  normal  cervical  sympathetic  trunk,  in- 
stead of  the  hypoglossal  nerve. 

The  converse  experiments,  those  of  cross-suturing  the  cervical  sym- 
pathetic trunk  into  the  hypoglossal  nerve,  were  carried  out  to  find  out 
if  the  larger  Schwann  tubes  of  the  distal  segment  would  allow  the  re- 


570  ANNALS  NEW  YORK  ACADEMY  OF  SCIENCES 

generating  fibers  to  expand  beyond  the  diameters  of  their  parent  fibers 
in  the  cervical  sympathetic  trunk.  The  results  showed  no  expansion, 
and  the  distal  fibers  tended  to  recover  the  characteristics  of  the  cervical 
sympathetic  trunk. 

DISCUSSION 

The  growth  of  fiber  diameter  is  part  of  a  process  of  reconstitution 
or  maturation  of  the  fibers  and  can  be  considered  separately  from  the 
longitudinal  growth  of  the  fibers  toward  the  periphery.  Although  this 
latter  process  of  outgrowth  has  been  the  subject  of  numerous  studies, 
the  diameter  growth  was  not  investigated  intensively  until  the  re- 
search of  Gutmann  and  Sanders^  on  rabbit  nerves.  They  described 
a  gradual  increase  in  fiber  diameter  for  the  first  year  of  regeneration 
after  suture,  without  recovery  of  either  the  maximum  diameter  or 
of  the  bimodal  fiber  distribution.  Our  experiments  confirm  these 
findings  and,  in  addition,  show  that  the  increase  in  diameter  con- 
tinues beyond  one  year  to  at  least  544  days.  Also,  at  extremely  long 
times,  up  to  1363  days  after  suture,  complete  recovery  of  diameter  is 
not  attained,  nor  is  the  bimodal  distribution  of  fibers.  The  fact  that 
crushing  allows  much  more  rapid  reconstitution  of  fiber  diameter  con- 
firms their  findings  (Gutmann  and  Sanders^). 

The  recovery  of  impulse  conduction  velocity  and  action  potential 
characteristics  is  also  an  important  part  of  this  reconstruction  or  ma- 
turation process  in  regenerating  nerve  fibers.  In  fact,  the  proper 
coordination  of  nerve  functions  might  be  impossible  if  their  conduction 
velocities  are  not  regained,  in  spite  of  proper  peripheral  connections. 
These  combined  electrical  and  microscopic  experiments  showed  that  the 
conduction  velocities  and  action  potentials  recover  slowly,  at  the  same 
rates,  as  the  fiber  diameters  increase.  The  conduction  velocities  were 
actually  compared  to  the  fiber  diameters  of  the  distal  stump,  and  the 
same  linear  relationship  between  these  two  functions  was  found  as  ex- 
pected for  normal  nerves,  as  reported  by  Gasser  and  Grundfest.^ 
Therefore,  the  electrical  characteristics  of  the  regenerating  outgrowths 
in  the  distal  stumps  were  found  to  be  related  solely  to  the  reconstitu- 
tion of  fiber  diameter  and  did  not  otherwise  depend  on  the  type  or 
size  of  the  parent  fiber  in  the  central  stump.  Further  data  on  the  ac- 
tion potentials  from  regenerating  nerves  have  been  reported  by  Berry, 
Grundfest,  and  Hinsey.'^ 

A  long  delay  between  sectioning  the  nerve  and  subsequent  suture 
was  shown  to  impede  the  usual  reconstitution  of  fiber  diameter  and  con- 


BERRY— HINSEY:  RECOVERY,  REGENERATING  NERVES      571 

duction  velocity.  Holmes  and  Young^  have  described  this  phenomenon 
and  have  shown  that  the  connective  tissue  tubes  in  the  distal  stump 
undergo  shrinkage  during  the  delay  period.  The  experiments  reported 
here  show  that  delay  causes  even  greater  influence  than  they  descril)ed. 
This  difference  in  results  is  probably  due  to  the  fact  that  our  experi- 
ments allowed  the  nerves  to  regenerate  for  much  longer  periods,  during 
which  the  restrictive  influences  could  be  more  strongly  exerted.  How- 
ever, the  results  show  that  brief  delay  periods  have  little  effect,  but  that 
delays  of  253  and  476  days  produced  considerable  interference  with 
fiber  reconstitution.  Unfortunately,  the  exact  delay  times  between 
no  effect  and  slight  effect  could  not  be  determined  from  these  experi- 
ments. The  introduction  of  control  cross-sutures,  without  delay  in 
nerves  of  the  opposite  leg  of  the  same  animals,  seems  to  rule  out  the 
factors  operating  at  the  suture  line,  or  differences  in  peripheral  re-in- 
nervation  (unless  atrophy  of  the  muscle  is  considered),  which  can  in- 
fluence the  fiber  reconstitution.  Presumably,  therefore,  only  differ- 
ences in  the  condition  of  the  connective  tissue  and  Schwann  tubes  of 
the  distal  stump  are  responsible  for  the  results  in  these  experiments. 

The  influence  of  the  connective  tissue  or  Schwann  tubes  in  the  distal 
stump  on  the  fiber  growth  has  been  recently  emphasized  by  Sanders  and 
Young,^  who  found  that  the  motor  branches  of  a  sutured,  mixed  nerve 
contained  fibers  of  larger  caliber  than  the  sensory  branches.  Also, 
Simpson  and  Young'*  cross-sutured  somatic  nerves  into  the  splanchnic 
and  anterior  mesenteric  nerves  and  suggested  that  the  restriction  in 
fiber  diameter  which  resulted  might  be  due,  in  part,  to  the  small  size 
of  the  peripheral  tubes.  The  results  reported  here  by  Hammond  and 
Hinsey''  showed  this  same  restriction  in  cross-sutures  of  the  hypoglossal 
and  cervical  sympathetic.  However,  in  these  experiments,  the  nerves 
were  allowed  to  regenerate  for  longer  periods  than  reported  by  Simpson 
and  Young,^  and  an  additional  phenomenon  was  disclosed.  At  216 
days  after  cross-suture  of  the  hypoglossal  to  the  cervical  sympathetic, 
the  recovery  of  fiber  diameter  was  slightly  less  than  that  obtained  in 
control,  hypoglossal-to-hypoglossal,  sutures.  At  250,  300,  and  365 
days,  the  fibers  not  only  showed  greater  restriction  of  growth,  but  actu- 
ally the  caliber  of  the  fibers  found  distally  became  smaller  than  they 
were  at  216  days.  There  is  no  conclusive  explanation  of  this  apparent 
reversal  of  diameter  growth,  but  two  facts  might  be  mentioned.  First, 
the  final  histogram  (figure  7),  at  365  days,  resembled  that  of  the  orig- 
inal distal  stump  before  operation,  and,  perhaps,  the  small  tubes  com- 
pressed or  killed  off  the  larger  fibers.  Secondly,  it  must  be  recognized 
that  the  hypoglossal  fibers  could  not  reach  proper  end  organs  by  grow- 


572  ANNALS  NEW  YORK  ACADEMY  OF  SCIENCES 

ing  down  the  cervical  sympathetic  trunk.  Simpson  and  Young^  showed 
the  importance  of  these  peripheral  connections  by  cutting  a  regener- 
ating nerve  peripheral  to  the  original  suture,  which  prevented  the  re- 
establishment  of  peripheral  connections.  Weiss  and  Taylor^  also 
found  evidence  that  fibers  were  smaller  when  re-innervation  of  the  end 
organs  was  prevented. 

SUMMARY 

1.  Excised,  distal  stumps  of  tibial,  peroneal,  and  saphenous  nerves  of 
cats  were  studied  oscillographically  and  microscopically,  at  intervals, 
up  to  1363  days  after  transection  and  suture. 

2.  The  processes  of  maturation  or  reconstitution  of  fiber  diameter 
and  impulse  conduction  velocity  continued  over  a  long  period  of  at 
least  544  days.     The  regenerating  fibers  never  completely  recovered. 

3.  Crushed  nerves  recovered  fiber  diameter  and  conduction  velocity 
more  rapidly  than  sutured  nerves. 

4.  Delay  between  transection  and  suture  of  more  than  6  months  in- 
terfered with  the  reconstitution  of  the  regenerating  fibers. 

5.  Cross-suturing  a  nerve  containing  large  fibers  into  a  distal  stump 
containing  small  connective  tissue  or  Schwann  tubes  resulted  in  re- 
striction of  fiber  diameter  growth. 

REFERENCES 

1.  Gutmann,  E.,  &  F.  K.  Sanders 

1943.  J.  Physiol.  101:489. 

2.  Weiss  P. 

1944.  '  J.  Nemo.surg.  1:  400. 

3.  Holmes,  W.,  &  J.  Z.  Young 

1942.     J.  Anat.  77:  63. 

4.  Simpson,  S.  A.,  &  J.  Z.  Young 

1945.  J.  Anat.  79:48. 

5.  Gasser,  H.  S.,  &  H.  Grundfest 
1939.     Am.  J.  Physiol.  127:  393. 

6.  Hammond,  W.  S.,  &  J.  C,  Hinsey 
194.').     J.  Comp.  Neurol.  83:  79. 

7.  Berry,  C.  M.,  H.  Grundfest,  &  J.  C.  Hinsey 
1944.     J.  Neurophysiol.  7:  103. 

8.  Sanders,  E.  K,,  &  J.  Z.  Young 
1944.     J.  Physiol.  103:  119. 

9.  Weiss,  P.,  &  A.  C.  Taylor 

1944.     J.  exp.  Zool.  95:233. 


BERRY—HLXSEY:  RECOVERY,  REGENERATING  NERVES      573 


PLATE  5 


574  ANNALS  NEW  YORK  ACADEMY  OF  SCIENCES 


Plate  5 

Action  potentials  recorded  from  excised  distal  stumps  of  regenerating  tibial 
nerves. 

A  was  recorded  after  36  days  of  regeneration  with  a  monopolar  electrode  placed 
3.5  cm.  distal  to  the  suture,  and  the  conduction  distance  was  3.5  cm.  The  spike 
is  approximately  25  microvolts,  and  the  time  signals  under  A  are  1.7  millisec.  per 
cycle. 

B  was  recorded  from  a  normal  tibial  nerve  with  a  conduction  distance  of  8  cm., 
and  B  is  a  control  record  for  C. 

C  is  from  the  opposite  regenerating  tibial,  1363  days  after  suture  with  the  same 
8  cm.  conduction  distance,  and  the  same  amplification  as  B. 
The  time  line  for  B  and  C  is  1  millisec.  per  cycle. 


Annals  N.  Y.  Ac\n.  Sci. 


Vol.  XLVII,  Aht.  i,  Platk 


\^  '^"M^s^s^w^ 


BERRY  AND  HINSEY;   RECOVERY  OF  CONDLTTION  IN   FIBERS 


NERVE  METABOLISM  AND  FUNCTION  = 
A  CRITIQUE  OF  THE  ROLE  OF  ACETYLCHOLINE 

By  R.  W.  Gerard 

Department  of  Physiology,  The  University  of  Chicago,  Chicago,  Illinois. 

INTRODUCTION 

Clearly,  the  acetylcholine  system  is  the  theme  around  which  these 
papers  have  been  arranged.  The  various  hypotheses  as  to  its  functional 
significance,  and  especially  the  one  regarding  it  as  an  essential  com- 
ponent in  conduction  in  the  nerve  fiber,  have  proven  most  fertile  in  re- 
search suggestions — witness  the  many  studies  here  reported  and  the 
animated  discussion  of  them.  Yet,  I  must  close  with  the  judg- 
ment, on  the  basis  of  what  has  been  said  here,  that  this  hypothesis  has 
now  exhausted  its  usefulness. 

May  I  first  offer,  as  evidence  of  my  own  long  sympathy  to  the' view  I 
shall  shortly  be  dissecting,  a  quotation  or  two  from  my  early  writings? 

"It  remains  to  correlate  this  material  [on  heat  and  metabolism]  with 
some  actual  mechanism  of  conduction.  The  current  view  that  activity 
of  one  portion  of  a  nerve  fiber  is  the  stimulus  to  the  adjacent  portion 
and  so  along  the  entire  fiber  has  much  to  support  it,  especially  in  the 
form  developed  by  Lillie.  Recent  evidence  indicates  that  conduction 
itself  may  be  analyzed  into  two  phases  occurring  repeatedly  in  succes- 
sion. The  first  is  an  explosive  type  of  chemical  change  in  a  portion 
of  the  membrane  surrounding  the  nerve  fiber,  and  it  leads,  probably 
by  local  potentials,  to  ion  movements  within  the  fiber,  which  constitute 
the  second  phase.  Local  concentration  of  ions  against  an  adjacent  por- 
tion of  membrane  initiates  here  the  explosive  change,  and  so  on.  Prob- 
ably the  ion  movements  are  associated  with  only  a  small  fraction  of  the 
energy  changes,  and  with  the  behavior  of  the  membrane  during  and 
after  conduction"  (P- 499^). 

"...  In  this  way,  it  is  obvious,  a  wave  of  electric  and  chemical  change 
must  spread  along  the  nerve  fiber  in  both  directions  from  the  point  first 
stimulated.  This  is  the  nerve  impulse,  a  propagated  excitation.  .  .  . 
Certain  steps  in  this  development  are  hypothetical,  and  it  must  be 
recognized  that  the  picture  has  been  simplified  to  a  merest  skeleton. 


*  This  paper  is  essentially  as  presented  on  February  9,   1946.    Later  developments  of  any  kind 
have  not  been  introduced  into  the  discussion. 

(575) 


576  ANNALS  NEW  YORK  ACADEMY  OF  SCIENCES 

Tlie  action  potential,  for  example,  may  not  represent  a  passive  de- 
polarization but  a  potential  actively  produced  by  the  chemical  reac- 
tions. But  whatever  the  details,  it  is  highly  probable  that  the  nerve 
impulse  consists  basically  of  a  local  membrane  change  of  a  chemical 
and  physical  nature,  which  leads  to  a  flow  of  ions,  or  current,  which 
in  turn  starts  the  local  membrane  change  at  adjacent  points"  (p.  64^). 

"...  Either  the  same  kind  of  ion  migration  and  chemical  response 
which  represents  successive  activation  of  one  region  of  the  nerve  fibre 
by  another  must  also  take  place  at  the  synapse,  or  it  is  conceivable 
that  the  end  of  the  axone  acts  as  a  miniature  gland  and,  when  stim- 
ulated, produces  some  chemical  which  is  able  to  excite  an  adjacent  or 
neighboring  dendrite"  (p.  74^) . 

"In  the  nervous  system  itself,  a  similar  mechanism  has  been  consid- 
ered by  several  workers.  The  end  of  an  axone  is  at  least  an  unspecial- 
ized  end  organ,  often  a  complicated  one  (as,  for  example,  in  the  olfac- 
tory glomeruli)  and  might  activate  the  dendrite  or  cell  body  on  which 
it  impinges  via  chemical  as  well  as  electrical  changes.  The  transmis- 
sion from  cell  to  cell  by  means  of  action  potentials  has  long  been  the 
orthodox  view,  and  emphasis  on  the  chemical  possibilities  has  had  a 
novel  flavor.  In  fact,  however,  the  conduction  along  a  nerve  fibre 
involves  excitation  of  a  resting  region  by  an  active  one,  and  both  elec- 
trical and  chemical  components  are  present  in  the  mechanism  of  propa- 
gation. At  the  ending,  which  is  specialized,  at  least  anatomically, 
either  or  both  components  might  well  be  exaggerated  to  facilitate  trans- 
mission over  a  critical  region.  Long-enduring  action  or  depolarization 
potentials  or  special  chemical  accumulation  might  equally  well  be 
utilized  in  various  situations  and  (except  for  familiarity  with  the  one 
idea)  one  seems  as  likely  as  the  other"  (p.  546^) . 

GENERAL   BACKGROUND 

The  Role  of  Metabolism 

Nerve  fibers,  like  whole  neurones  or  any  other  cells,  depend  on  a 
maintained  metabolism  to  survive  and  to  function.  This  was  strongly 
indicated  when  it  was  found,'*  near  the  start  of  this  century,  that  nerve 
conduction  failed  in  the  absence  of  oxygen;  and  was  proved  when 
nerve  respiration  and  heat  production,  at  rest  and  on  activity,  were 
successfully  measured  by  several  workers  in  the  mid-twenties.'"*-  ^  The 
next  question  is:  For  what  result  is  metabolism  essential?  Or,  What 
agencies  link  the  chemical  reactions  with  the  physiological  conse- 
quences?    In  general,  the  answer  is  clear  enough:  Metabolism  liberates 


GERARD:  NERVE  METABOLISM  AND  FUNCTION  577 

energy  to  do  necessary  work,  such  as  to  maintain  polarization  across 
a  leaky  membrane,  or  it  removes  unwanted  substances,  or  produces  re- 
(lui)'ed  ones.  Further,  the  change  in  concentration  of  a  substance  may 
or  may  not  be  a  needed  step  in  the  event  of  functioning,  an  indis- 
pensable gear  in  the  cell  machine. 

In  a  particular  case,  it  is  often  a  teasing  problem  to  determine  just 
what  role  a  metabolic  event  plays  in  a  tissue's  function.  The  formation 
of  lactate  in  muscle  contraction  is  a  perfect  illustration.  When  this 
relation  was  first  established,  early  this  century,  lactic  acid  was  at  once 
assigned  the  key  role  of  initiating  shortening.  It  was  an  essential  gear 
and,  perhaps  by  changing  surface  tension  due  to  acidity,  engaged  the 
shortening  mechanism.  Its  removal  or  neutralization  permitted  re- 
laxation. Later,  attention  to  energy  balance  emphasized  that  glycolysis 
could  supply  the  energy  required  in  anaerobic  contraction  and  that  this 
reaction  was  largely  rewound  with  oxidative  metabolism  in  oxygen. 
It  was  an  easy  assumption,  then,  even  under  aerobic  conditions  when 
no  lactate  change  was  found,  that  there  was  a  rapid  formation  and 
destruction  of  this  substance.  Indeed,  lactic  acid  was  considered  the 
essential  link  between  metabolism,  of  which  it  was  a  necessaiy  inter- 
mediate, and  contraction,  of  which  it  was  a  necessary  cause,  and  it 
was  supposedly  involved  in  both  energetics  and  mechanics. 

lodoacetic  acid,  alactic  contractions,  phosphocreatin  and  adenosine 
triphosphate  changes,  and,  finally,  the  use  of  lipid  fuels  (not  to  mention 
myosin) ,  changed  all  that.*^  Muscle  did  not  require  lactic  or  any  other 
acid  to  shorten  it ;  lactate  is  not  part  of  the  machinery.  The  CrP  and 
ATP  breakdown  supplied  the  early  energy  needed  for  contraction,  heat, 
and  work;  lactate  formation  is  not  an  immediate  energy  source.  Mod- 
erate exercise  with  good  oxygenation  involved  no  lactate  change  and 
little  carbohydrate  loss;  lactate  is  in  no  way  necessary  to  contraction. 
It  is  just  one  of  the  many  initial  or  intermediate  fuels  available  to  the 
engine  under  normal  working  conditions,  and  its  accumulation  anaero- 
bically  is,  in  a  sense,  a  sign  of  failure  to  complete  the  initiated 
oxidations. 

In  a  particular  case,  further,  it  is  well  to  note  that  historical  acci- 
dents greatly  influence  the  trend  of  our  scientific  thought  and  research. 
Acetylcholine  first  came  to  attention  as  a  pharmacologic  agent;  ATP, 
as  an  intracellular  substance  involved  in  important  metabolic  sequences. 
The  great  experimental  sweeps  were,  accordingly,  oriented  differently 
in  the  two  cases.  Yet  ATP  also  has  profound  pharmacological  ac- 
tions,'- **•  *•  and  ACh  may  well  prove  to  be  an  important  compon^ 
cell  metabolic  systems  in  general.  This  point  will  require 
later. 


578  ANNALS  NEW  YORK  ACADEMY  OF  SCIENCES 

Neural  Metabolism 

Finally,  I  shall  recall  the  gross  metabolic  picture  of  neural 
tissue. ^""^^  For  nerve,  in  contrast  to  muscle,  the  heat  and  respira- 
tion balance  of  rest  and  action  were  first  established.  Lactic  acid, 
next  studied,  seemed  to  be  excluded  from  any  role  except  as  an 
anaerobic  end-product,  but  it  was  later  shown  to  serve  as  an  effective 
substitute  fuel  when  sugar  oxidation  was  interfered  with  by  iodoacetic 
acid;  and  anaerobic  glycolysis  was  similarly  established  as  a  source  of 
useful  energy.  Yet  oxidations,  by  oxygen  or  an  oxidizing  reserve,  re- 
main of  especial  importance  in  nerve,  for  the  long  and  large  delayed 
heat  production  cannot  be  dissociated  from  the  initial  heat  by  anoxia 
or  by  any  other  maneuver  tried.  What  fuel  or  fuels  are  oxidized,  is 
largely  unknown.  At  rest,  nerve  may  destroy  more  carbohydrate  than 
could  be  fully  oxidized,  while  the  R.  Q.  hovers  at  0.8;  and  CHO  utiliza- 
tion can  taper  off  to  zero  while  O2  consumption  is  maintained  unaltered. 
Even  brain,  with  a  resting  R.  Q.  of  1.0  and  a  CHO  fuel,  can  shift  to 
another  substrate  which  fully  supports  respiration.  During  activity, 
the  much  increased  oxidative  metabolism  of  nerve  is  not  supported  by 
CHO.  The  R.  Q.  of  the  extra  respiration  does  rise  to  nearly  1.0  in 
tetanized  nerves,  but  CHO  loss  is  not  increased.  Some  rise  in  acid- 
soluble  phosphorus  and  in  ammonia-liberation  occurs,  suggesting  the 
degradation  of  phospholipins  or  phosphoproteins;  but  the  problem  is 
still  wide  open.  The  lipo-protein  changes  in  rods  on  illumination 
(mentioned  by  Wald)  and  the  Swedish  work^^  on  nucleoprotein  de- 
crease in  fatigued  nerve  (to  which  Schmitt  called  attention),  are  ob- 
servations challenging  a  resolution  of  this  enduring  uncertainty. 

THE  NERVE  MACHINE 

The  resting  metabolism  of  nerve  is  essential  to  keeping  the  tissue 
functionable.  The  normal  resting  potential,  for  example,  falls  when 
respiration  is  prevented^''  and  even  more  rapidly  when  glycolysis  is 
also  blocked. ^^  When  an  action  is  evoked,  the  cell  machinery  whirs, 
physical  and  chemical  changes  occur,  an  impulse  is  propagated,  and, 
finally,  a  cycle  is  completed  and  the  machine  fully  reset.  The  events 
associated  with  activity  are  known  in  moderate  detail,  and  it  will  be 
helpful  to  outline  this  sequence.  Since  so  much  attention  has  been 
given  by  investigators  to  the  early  and  the  electrical  phenomena  of 
response,  may  I  emphasize  that  all  the  phenomena  are  closely  coupled 
together.  A  single  impulse,  gone  by  in  a  millisecond,  is  yet  irrevocably 
followed  by  a  rise  in  heat  liberation  and  in  oxygen  consumption  which 


GERARD:  NERVE  METABOLISM  AND  FUNCTION  579 

endure  for  minutes.  Conduction  fails  when  respiration^'  ^^  or  gly- 
colysis^^ is  disturbed,  although,  whether  this  is  a  result  of  interference 
directly  with  active  metabolism,  or  is  secondary  to  interference  with 
resting  metabolism,  is  not  clear. •"  A  mechanism,  partly  in  terms  of 
phosphate  intermediates,  for  insuring  the  one-to-one  relation  between 
early  and  late  events,  was  suggested  some  time  ago-°  and  is  still  useful. 
But,  before  pursuing  this  aspect,  what  of  conduction  itself? 

Depolarization 

Electric  currents,  applied  to  nerve  or  muscle,  excite  at  the  cathode, 
where  ion  movements  are  such  as  to  depolarize  the  polarized  mem- 
brane. The  most  direct  evidence  for  the  preexisting  membrane  poten- 
tial and  for  its  diminution  by  trans-membrane  currents  is  that  from 
impaled  single  ncrve"^' '^  and  muscle  fibers. ^^  Membrane  potentials 
up  to  £0  mV.  have  been  obtained  from  resting  units;  and  excitation 
is  easily  achieved  with  a  cathode  outside  and  anode  inside  the  fiber, 
but  even  100- fold  greater  currents  in  the  reverse  direction  are  ineffec- 
tive. Further,  recalling  the  uniquely  high  sensitivity  of  these  tissues 
to  electric  currents  and  the  generation  of  electric  changes  when  non- 
electric stimuli  are  applied,  it  seems  probable  that  membrane  depolar- 
ization by  ion  movements  is  the  initial  step  in  all  forms  of  natural 
excitation  of  nerve  and  muscle.  AVhether  excitation  results  most  di- 
rectly from  a  potential,  impedance,  or  other,  change,  and  to  what  crit- 
ical level,  is  a  separate  and  secondary  problem. 

Active  Membrane  Participation 

There  is  much  evidence  that  the  nerve  membrane  does  not  passively 
follow  the  imposed  depolarization,  at  least  when  applied  currents  are 
more  than  a  few  per  cent  of  threshold,  but  responds  with  active  changes. 
These  changes  are  almost  certainly  chemical  as  well  as  physical.  The 
decreased  impedance  is  suggestive,  but  perhaps  not  convincing,  on  this 
point.  The  existence  of  prepotentials  (with  depolarizing  shocks,  but 
not  with  equal  ones  in  the  reverse  direction)  in  invertebrate^*-  ^^  and 
vertebrate  nerve^^  has  been  several  times  referred  to  in  this  publication. 
The  fact  that  these  often  oscillate,  and  that  the  oscillations  can  incre- 
ment without  additional  external  change,-*  has  been  emphasized  here 
by  the  report  of  Bronk  and  Brink,  and  by  Cole's  discussion.  The  oscil- 
lation period,  4  to  5  msec,  observed  in  Ca-depleted  nerves  (Bronk), 
fits  satisfactorily  with  the  physical  constants  of  the  membrane,  men- 
tioned by  Curtis,  which  should  lead  to  resonance  at  about  250  cycles 
per  sec.     But  such  physical  factors  control  only  the  period  of  oscilla- 


580  ANNALS  NEW  YORK  ACADEMY  OF  SCIENCES 

tion,  and  cannot  supply  the  energy  to  maintain,  even  less  to  increase, 
it.  Energy,  presumably  liberated  from  metabolic  events,  must  supply 
the  drive,  while  physical  conditions  only  modulate  its  flow.  The  analogy 
used  in  connection  with  the  similar  problem  of  electric  oscillations  in 
the  isolated  frog  brain-'  may  be  used  again.  Air  pressure,  from  the 
motor,  drives  most  windshield  wipers,  and  their  beat  does  rise  and 
fall  with  this;  but  the  beat  is  much  more  under  the  control  of  a  valve, 
which  determines  when  each  stroke  is  tripped  off. 

Another  set  of  facts  bespeaks,  even  more  strongly,  the  intervention 
of  a  chemical  step  this  early  in  the  excitation  process.  In  tortoise 
auricle,^^  crab  nerve, 2*^  and  even  frog  nerve--'  (see  also  Gerard,^"  and 
following  discussion),  an  opposed  electric  shock,  delivered  between  a 
supra-threshold  shock  and  the  start  of  the  resulting  propagated  re- 
sponse, can  nullify  the  response.  While  it  may  be  possible  for  the 
purely  physical  changes,  produced  by  a  pulse  in  an  appropriate  network, 
to  surge  on  to  a  peak  after  the  pulse  has  passed  (as  Curtis  suggested 
in  connection  with  the  reversed  action  potential),  such  a  physical  in- 
terpretation is  under  the  burden  of  offering  positive  evidence  in  the 
case  of  the  cooled  auricle,  where  a  reverse  shock  given  20  msec,  after 
an  effective  one  is  still  able  to  abort  the  response. 

The  Discontinuous  Response 

When  the  local  membrane  changes  have  progressed  sufficiently,  a  full- 
fledged  action  appears  and  propagates.  This  response,  as  several 
speakers  have  emphasized,  is  not  a  continuation  of  the  earlier  processes, 
but  a  new  and  explosive  group  of  events.  Here,  even  more  surely  than 
in  the  preceding  phase,  chemical  as  well  as  physical  changes  are  in- 
volved. The  resting  membrane  potential  shifts  abruptly,  not  merely 
toward  or  to  neutrality,  but  to  an  inverted  magnitude  which  can  much 
exceed  the  original  level. ^^'  ">  ^^  Perhaps,  as  Curtis  suggests,  this  is 
only  a  physical  overshoot,  rather  than  a  newly-developed,  oppositely- 
oriented,  and  chemically-active  membrane  battery;  but  the  burden  of 
proof  seems  to  be  clearly  on  the  adherents  to  such  a  physical  view. 
Cole's  comment,  that  the  reversed  action  potential  can  vary  in  mag- 
nitude independently  of  the  resting  potential,  certainly  favors  more 
the  positive  conclusion.  Hober's  suggestion,  that  a  fatty  acid  is  re- 
leased by  activated  lecithinase  and,  reaching  the  inside  of  the  mem- 
brane, reverses  its  potential,  just  as  caproic  acid  does  when  placed 
on  the  outside,  is  an  example  of  the  chemical,  active-membrane-change 
viewpoint.  (This  particular  example  is  not  fully  satisfying,  however; 
for,  if  the  non-polar  chains  enter  the  membrane  lipids  and  the  polar 


GERARD:    NERVE   METABOLISM   AND   FUNCTION  581 

earboxyl  groups  form  a  negatively  charged  layer  in  the  aqueous  phase, 
this  could  shift  the  outer  membrane  surface  charge  from  positive  to 
negative,  but  could  hardly  shift  the  inner  surface  charge  from  nega- 
tive to  positive.) 

The  well-known  high  temperature  coefficients  of  excitation  also  speak, 
though  admittedly  in  an  uncertain  voice,  for  chemical  components  in 
the  process.  If  elongated  molecules  in  a  loose  palisade  in  the  mem- 
brane are  merely  bent  about,  during  stimulation,  then  they  must  make 
quite  a  sudden  fall  when  a  threshold  is  reached,  and  must  also  start  a 
vigorous  series  of  changes.  For,  whether  the  main  chemical  reactions 
of  metabolism  accelerate  during,  or  only  after,  the  explosive  membrane 
response,  they  are  locked  to  it  in  an  essentially  invariable  sequence. 
And,  finally,  the  important  and  complex  impedance,  and  potential, 
variations  which  accompany  or  follow  the  spike  surely  indicate  proc- 
esses beyond  simple  ion  movements  or  dielectric  strains.  The  action 
potential  spike  represents  more  than  a  passive  depolarization  of  a  pre- 
viously charged  membrane.  It  is  an  active  physico-chemical  process, 
still  unknown  in  its  details. 

Local  Currents 

Whatever  the  events  in  an  activated  membrane  region,  there  remains 
no  doubt  as  to  the  mechanism  of  projjagation  along  a  nerve  or  muscle 
fiber.  Voltage  differences  between  active  and  not-yet-active  areas 
must  lead  to  current  flow  between  them  and  to  catelectronic  depolariza- 
tion of  the  latter.  That  such  currents  are  a  sufficient  mechanism  for 
propagation  is  certain  from  the  experiments  in  which  the  nerve  im- 
pulse is  made  to  jump  a  block  one  or  two  millimeters  Iqng.^^-  ^'^'  ^^  Even 
normally,  propagation  is  probably  by  similar  saltations  from  node  to 
node,  in  medullated  fibers.'*'  '^^'  ^® 

Immediate  Recovery 

During  the  absolute  refractory  period,  often  under  a  millisecond,  the 
membrane  must  at  least  recover  toward  its  normal  potential,  impedance, 
and  other  properties,  so  that  it  is  again  activable.  While  the  anodal 
action  of  the  eddy  currents  sweeping  on  ahead  may  contribute  to  this 
restoration,  this  is  obviously  insufficient.  Energy  has  been  dissipated 
and  must  be  made  good  from  sources  beyond  the  currents  which  help 
dissipate  it.  There  can  be  no  reasonable  doubt  that  the  complex  of 
initial  and  immediately-subsequent  recovery,  with  the  reversing  thresh- 
olds and  potentials  already  well  known,''  is  dependent  on  one  or  more 
of  the  energj^-yielding  metabolic  reactions;  perhaps  on  ATP  breakdown. 


582  ANNALS  NEW  YORK  ACADEMY  OF  SCIENCES 

Full  Restoration 

For  completeness,  although  it  is  far  removed  from  the  direct  problem 
of  propagation,  I  mention,  finally,  the  delayed  recovery  processes.  We 
often  forget  that  the  increased  respiration  of  activity  persists  a  half- 
hour  or  more  after  a  brief  tetanus  of  nerve,^^  that  the  delayed  heat 
production  is  similarly  prolonged,^"  and  that  considerable  after-poten- 
tials may  endure  for  comparable  periods.''"  And,  as  late  recovery  lags 
progressively  further  behind  in  a  continuously-driven  nerve,  its  re- 
sponse capacity  falls  to  a  lower  equilibrium  level.  Irritability,  velocity, 
chemical,  thermal,  and  electrical  response  per  impulse,  etc.,  fall  during 
a  maintained  tetanization.'*^ 

Now,  with  this  outline  of  nerve  action  before  us,  I  should  like  to  con- 
sider the  questions  around  which  so  much  of  this  symposium  has  re- 
volved: (1)  How  does  excitation  engage  metabolism;  (2)  what  is  the 
roh  of  the  acetylcholine  system;  and  (3)  in  what  respects  does  junc- 
tional transmission  differ  from  that  in  a  fiber? 

THE  LINKAGE  OF  ACTION  TO  METABOLISM 

Given  the  externally-applied  stimulus  energy,  given  even  the  propa- 
gated membrane  response,  the  insistent  question  remains  as  to  how  one 
event  induces  the  next  and,  especially,  how  chemical  changes  are  made 
to  follow  the  physical  ones.  This  was  asked  by  Grundfest,  discussed 
by  Green,  and  exemplified  by  Ochoa's  contribution.  It  was  considered 
for  nerve,  in  some  detail,  a  decade  ago,^''  and  is  today  being  clarified  in 
the  case  of  muscle.*^  In  muscle,  the  extra  step  of  mechanical  response 
offers  both  an  additional  problem,  of  how  the  membrane  response  leads 
to  the  myosin  response,  and  an  additional  line  of  attack  on  the  general 
case.  Since  metabolic  details  are  far  more  numerous  for  muscle  than 
for  nerve,  I  shall  choose  illustrations  freely  also  from  the  former 
material,  in  confidence  that  the  principles  they  illustrate  are  equally 
valid  for  both  tissues. 

Ion  Action 

In  the  homogeneous  liquid  phase  of  a  heterogeneous  system  like  tis- 
sue, electric  currents,  applied  as  external  stimuli  or  generated  in  the 
course  of  the  active  response,  mean  ion  movements  and  only  ion  move- 
ments. Where  these  ion  streams  encounter  interfaces — membranes, 
micelles,  molecular  palisades,  etc. — ions  can  accumulate  or  decrease. 
A  local  change  in  ion  concentration  at  molecular  or  structural  surfaces 
of  a  cell  must  be  the  initial  consequence  of  an  electric  stimulus  and  ap- 


GERARD:  NERVE  METABOLISM  AND   FUNCTION  583 

pears  to  be  the  only  possible  first  link  to  a  metabolic  chain  of  events. 
(Electron  shifts  within  single  molecules  or  lattices,  if  such  occur,  would 
themselves  follow  the  shift  of  charged  ions,  unless  large  electromagnetic 
fields  were  applied— fields  that  seem  beyond  a  range  of  possible  bio- 
logical significance.)  However,  changed  ion  concentration  is  easily 
sufficient  to  initiate  other  local  chemical  changes. 

Altered  metabolism  means  changes  in  the  rates  of  chemical  reactions. 
Not  only  are  quantitative  increases  or  decreases  of  total  metabolism  the 
sum  of  similar  variations  in  the  rates  of  the  component  reactions;  but 
also  qualitative  changes  are  the  resultant  of  increase  in  rates  of  certain 
reactions  and  decreases  in  others.  The  rate  of  a  given  reaction  is 
determined  by  the  concentration  of  active  reactants  and  products  and 
by  the  catalytic  conditions  (temperature,  water,  ions,  etc.),  especially 
by  the  enzyme  activity.  Reactant  concentrations  can  change  only 
as  a  result  of  an  antecedent  change  in  another  chemical  system  which 
produces  them — which  gets  us  no  further  in  our  problem — or  of  a 
spatial  redistribution.  If  reactants  are  themselves  ions,  and  so  moved 
by  the  stimulating  currents,  this  could  be  a  direct  result  of  stimulation. 
In  most  cases,  however,  as  emphasized  by  Hober,  such  a  redistribution 
would  also  demand  prior  changes  in  the  system  to  increase  the  physical 
availability,  changes  in  membrane  barriers,  surface  adsorption,  and  the 
like.  These  might  also  be  a  direct  effect  of  the  stimulus,  as  the  rota- 
tion of  a  polar  molecule,  but  are  more  likely  to  be  secondary  to  more 
extensive  chemical  changes. 

In  contrast  to  the  relatively  unpromising  situation  for  substrate  al- 
teration, a  modification  of  enzyme  activity,  and  so  of  metabolism,  by 
ion  changes  is  both  theoretically  probable  and  experimentally  estab- 
lished. Besides  a  direct  ion  effect  on  the  activity  of  given  enzyme 
molecules,  there  exist  the  other  possibilities  of  activating  pro-enzymes 
(Ca  on  prothrombin),  removing  inhibitors  (phosphate  or  citrate  bind- 
ing calcium),  and  adding  accelerators  (Cu  on  thiol  oxidation*^).  Such 
ion  effects  are  richly  present  in  biological  systems,  as  well  as  in  the 
non-living  systems  mentioned  by  Dr.  Alexander,  and  it  may  be  useful 
to  itemize  some  that  are  important  in  muscle  and  nerve  tissiies.^^'  ^^ 

Magnesium  ion  either  is  essential  to,  or  materially  hastens,  a  number 
of  key  reactions  in  carbohydrate  degradation,  while  local  increase  in  its 
concentration  would  suffice  to  initiate  or  accelerate  them.  The  phos- 
phorylation of  glucose  to  hexose-6-phosphate  by  hexokinase,  a  reaction 
of  especial  importance  in  neural  tissue  which  "prefers"  glucose  to  glyco- 
gen as  a  fuel,  requires  Mg""*;  as  does,  also,  the  shift  of  phosphate  from 
the  1  to  6  position  by  glucophosphomutase.^'*    The  further  phosphory- 


584  ANNALS  NEW  YORK  ACADEMY  OF  SCIENCES 

lation  of  this  substance,  an  intermediate  in  the  glycogen  as  well  as 
glucose  reaction  chain,  to  fructose  1,6-diphosphate,  also  requires  Mg^*. 
The  later  change  in  phosphoglyceric  acid  to  the  energy-rich  phos- 
phoenol-pyruvic  acid  involves  a  magnesium  combination  with  enolase, 
and  fluoride  inhibition  of  this  reaction  depends  on  displacement  of 
Mg^*  by  a  fluoride  complex. ^^  Magnesium  or  manganese,  as  well  as 
calcium  (K  inhibits),  is  an  essential  component  of  the  system  which 
forms  ATP  and  pyruvic  acid  from  the  phosphopyruvic  acid  and  a  lower 
adenosin  phosphate.*"  This  ion  is  again  reported  necessary  for  the 
splitting  of  ATP  to  ADP  by  myosin,^'  although  most  workers*^'  *''•  ^° 
find  Mg"""^  inhibitory  here.  Mg*""  also  inhibits  the  shift  from  3-phos- 
phoglyceric  acid  to  2-phosphoglyceric  acid  by  phosphotriose  mutase.^^ 

Calcium  ion,  as  mentioned,  is  required  for  the  formation  of  ATP 
from  phosphopyruvic  acid.  It  is  also  involved  in  splitting  a  phosphate 
from  ATP  by  myosin  or  other  ATP-ases'*^'  ^'^'  -'^  and,  perhaps,  in  the 
accompanying  shortening  of  the  myosin  fibers.  The  splitting  of  acetyl- 
phosphate  to  acetate  and  phosphate  is  accelerated  by  Ca"^"^.^*  And, 
again,  to  mention  an  inhibitory  action,  Ca""*  interferes  with  the  forma- 
tion of  acetylcholine  from  choline. ^^ 

The  formation  of  ATP  is  thus  influenced  by  all  three  of  the  major 
cellular  cations  (Mg,  Ca,  K) ;  its  destruction,  by  at  least  two  (Mg,  Ca). 
ATP,  in  turn,  is  critical  in  both  fat  and  carbohvdrate  oxidation  and 
may  be  one  of  the  regulators  of  metabolism.  Thus,  a  lowered  ATP 
concentration  might  favor  utilization  of  carbohydrate  over  fat'^*'  and 
glycolysis  over  respiration.'^'  The  abrupt  shift  of  muscle  metabolism, 
on  vigorous  contraction,  in  just  these  directions,  may.  then,  be  due 
to  the  fall  in  ATP  concentration  and  this,  in  turn,  to  the  movement  of 
ions  to  or  from  the  critical  enzyme  surfaces. 

Another  example  of  ion  importance,  especially  of  K+,  is  offered  by 
recent  myosin  studies.  In  an  appropriate  system,  myosin  B  contracts 
in  0.1  M  KCl  and  relaxes  when  the  K""  is  doubled  in  concentration^*; 
and  an  antagonism  between  K"^  and  Ca*+,  or  Mg^^  and  Ca""*,  on  myosin 
action  as  ATP-ase  and  on  myosin  extension,  has  been  repeatedly  noted. 
Potassium  also  increases  the  content  of  creatin  phosphate  in  muscle 
(while  Ca"^""  decreases  it),^"'  •^"'  ^"  as  well  as  of  stable  phosphate  esters 
in  nerve,**^  perhaps  by  its  ability  to  enhance  CrP  formation  when 
pyruvic  acid  is  oxidized.''-  This  ion  also  aids  both  the  synthesis  and 
the  liberation  of  ACh  and,  conversely,  ACh    (or  ATP)    can  release 

Tr+  63,  .55,  64 

It  is  not  difficult  to  trace  connections  between  these  catalytic  actions 
of  the  tissue  cations  and  the  physiologic  effects  which  ion  changes  pro- 


GERARD:   NERVE  METABOLISM  AND   FUNCTION  585 

duce;  but,  at  present,  relating  specific  actions  to  specific  effects  would 
be  mainly  guesswork.  It  will  suffice  to  recall  that  K^  increase  leads 
to  such  effects  as:  a  rise,  passing  into  a  severe  fall,  for  the  irritability, 
membrane  potential,  and  electrotonic  spread  in  nerve  fibers,  and,  per- 
haps, for  their  oxygen  consumption,  as  Brink  mentioned;  and  a  fall, 
from  the  start,  for  the  spike  (only  slight),  after-potential,  conduction 
velocity,  and  recovery  rate.  Similar  changes  have  been  observed 
less  fully  in  muscle  and  in  the  central  nervous  system:  moderately  in- 
creased K+,  for  example,^^'  2''  '^^  increases  the  fast  electrical  activity 
of  cat  or  frog  brain  and  prolongs  the  after-discharge  on  stimulation  of 
deep  cerebellar  nuclei  (see  also  ^^) .  There  are,  thus,  ample  roads  from 
current  flow,  through  altered  ion  concentration  and  chemical  reaction 
rates,  to  physiological  responses.  The  problem  is  not  to  find  connec- 
tions, but  rather  to  identify  the  few  important  actualities  among  the 
many  conceivable  possibilities.     This  brings  us  back  to  acetylcholine. 

THE   ROLE   OF    THE    ACETYLCHOLINE    SYSTEM 

That  ACh  is  formed  and  destroyed  as  an  integral  part  of  impulse 
propagation  in  nerve  fibers,  has  been  suggested  by  several  workers"^'  '^^ 
and  strongly  supported  by  Nachmansohn.*'"  He  has  summarized  his 
arguments  here :  ACh  is  present  in  nerve  and  is  released  on  stimulation 
(though  its  leaving  the  cell  is  accidental)  ;  cholinesterase  (ChE)  in 
nerve  is  highly  active  and  specific,  and  choline  acetylase  (ChA)  is 
also  rich  in  nerve;  a  close  parallelism  exists,  in  the  electric  organ,  be- 
tween potential  and  ChE  activity;  energy  relations,  in  electric  organ 
and  nerve,  are  satisfactory  for  ACh  synthesis  via  CrP,  etc.;  various 
drug  actions,  though  always  in  danger  of  misinterpretation,  especially 
where  penetration  through  a  membrane  is  involved,  do  support  the  im- 
portance of  ACh  in  conduction.  Just  what  the  role  of  ACh  is,  seems 
less  defined.  Earlier,  Nachmansohn  supposed  that  the  stimulus  liber- 
ated ACh  directly  and  that  this  caused  the  membrane  depolarization. 
Now,  recognizing  that  the  stimulus  itself  must  lead  to  depolarization, 
he  suggests  that  ACh  is  responsible  for  the  loss  of  resistance  in  the 
membrane — certainly,  a  step  for  which  a  chemical  mechanism  would 
be  welcome.  Beutner  and  Barnes  have  also  emphasized  a  function 
for  ACh,  both  in  producing  the  action  potential  and  in  lowering  mem- 
brane resistance. 

Quantitative  Relations 

The  calculations  (Nachmansohn),  that  the  ChE  at  a  motor  end- 
plate  is  powerful  enough  to  split  a  complete  layer  of  ACh  in  a  milli- 


586  ANNALS  NEW  YORK  ACADEMY  OF  SCIENCES 

second  (perhaps  even  in  a  few  microseconds,  which  Cole  has  mentioned 
as  a  more  appropriate  time  for  the  impedance  changes  in  nerve),  and 
that  the  enzyme  activity  in  the  C.N.S.  would  split  a  layer  covering 
10^  sq.  mu.  per  gm.,  are  impressive.  There  is  no  question  of  strong 
esterase  activity.  However,  it  is  worth  noting,  for  the  C.N.S.,  that 
even  the  surface  of  the  nuclei  in  a  gram  of  brain,  let  alone  the  whole 
neuronal  surface,  is  some  4  X  10^°  sq.  mu.'^''  The  difficulties  appear 
when  one  examines  the  rest  of  the  system,  to  see  how  well  it  can  keep 
'up  with  the  esterase.  Let  us  assume,  with  Nachmansohn,  that  some 
2000  cal/M  are  required  to  esterify  choline  to  ACh,  and  calculate,  from 
his  data  on  enzyme  content,  Feldberg's  summary'^  of  ACh  content  and 
liberation,  and  the  figures  of  myself  and  others  on  heat  and  metab- 
olism, the  over-all  balance  for  nerve  and  brain. 

Nachmansohn  estimates  that,  in  mammalian  brain,  ChE  can  split  up 
to  10"^  molecules  of  ACh  per  millisecond  per  gram  fresh  tissue.  This 
amounts  to  6  millimoles  per  hour  per  gram.  In  terms  of  Qcit:  values, 
reduced  to  these  same  units  (mM,  hr,  gm.),  less  ACh  could  be  split: 
between  0.3  mM  for  cortex,  and  3.0  for  caudate  nucleus  or  sympathetic 
ganglia.  For  mammalian  nerve,  the  rate  calculates  to  0.06;  for  frog 
nerve  (20°),  to  0.05;  and  for  white  matter,  to  0.02.  In  contrast,  the 
maximum  rate  of  ACh  synthesis  (in  tissue  brei  in  N.,  with  ATP  and 
all  necessary  accessories)  is  0.001  mM  for  mammalian  brain  and  0.0005 
for  nerve,  still  in  these  same  units.  In  both  mammalian  brain  and 
nerve,  therefore,  ChE  activity  is  over  1000  times,  perhaps  over  5000 
times,  as  great  as  ChA  activity,  and  similar  relations  will  probably  be 
found  for  the  frog.  Neural  enzymes  can  split  ACh  by  three  or  four 
magnitudes  faster  than  they  can  build  it. 

This  calculation  is  made,  of  course,  for  maximum  rates  and  over  long 
time  intervals,  and  requires  further  consideration.  If,  for  example,  the 
synthesis  normally  continues  evenly  in  time,  but  the  hydrolysis  occurs 
only  in  brief  bursts  associated  with  activity,  the  discrepancy  in  rates 
might  be  unimportant.  But  this  will  not  hold.  First,  whether  ACh  be 
associated  with  the  potential  or  impedance  changes  of  a  nerve  action, 
the  rise  is  far  more  rapid  than  the  fall,  and  the  need  for  an  explosive 
release  of  the  agent  is  even  more  imperative  than  for  an  explosive  de- 
struction. If,  therefore,  ACh  is  synthesized  and  destroyed  in  the 
course  of  each  nerve  action,  ChA  should  actually  be  several-fold  more 
active  than  ChE,  instead  of  a  thousand-fold  less  active.  Let  us  make 
the  more  favorable  assumption,  however,  that  ACh  need  not  actually 
be  synthesized  for  each  impulse,  but  only  be  released  from  a  store. 
Then,  though  used  in  bursts,  its  formation  could  be  continuous.  Even 
so,  there  remain  fatal  discrepancies. 


GERARD:  NERVE  METABOLISM  AND  FUNCTION  587 

The  total  ACh  present  in  whole  brain  is,  keeping  to  millimoles  per 
gram  fresh  tissue,  about  2  X  10"^  for  mammals  and  twice  as  much  for 
the  frog.  White  matter  contains  ten-fold  less,  but  the  value  for  mam- 
malian mixed  nerve  is  close  to  3  X  10'^.  Dorsal  roots  contain,  at  most, 
one-twentieth  of  this  amount;  frog  nerve,"'  even  less,  10~®.  All  the 
ACh  in  mammalian  brain  could,  therefore,  be  destroyed  by  ChE  in 
about  50  milliseconds,  and  would  require  a  minute  to  be  synthesized 
by  ChA.  For  mammalian  nerve,  the  stored  ACh  could  last  less  than 
two  seconds  and  would  require  over  three  minutes  to  replace ;  for  frog 
nerve,  the  ACh  could  last  about  65  milliseconds,  and  for  mammalian 
white  matter  (not  to  mention  dorsal  root),  a  third  of  a  second.  Yet 
nerve,  including  roots  and  central  tracts,  can  maintain  activity  for 
hours,  conducting  hundreds  of  impulses  per  second ;  and  activity  of  the 
central  grey  can  also  long  outlast  the  possible  time  limits.  Clearly, 
then,  neither  ACh  storage  nor  synthesis,  nor  both  combined,  could 
possibly  (unless  an  entirely  different  order  of  ChA  exists  in  vivo  than 
has  been  found  in  extracts)  supply  this  substrate  as  fast  as  ChE  can 
split  it. 

Of  course,  an  enzyme  is  not  always  kept  saturated  with  substrate. 
However,  this  at  once  undermines  the  many  arguments  that  have  been 
made,  from  high  ChE  concentration,  for  the  possibility  of  rapid  rise  and 
fall  of  ACh  concentration;  it  also  throws  into  question  the  significance 
of  high  local  ChE  concentrations.  The  striking  finding,  for  example, 
that  ChE  is  15,000  times  or  more  as  concentrated  in  the  end-plate  re- 
gion as  in  the  adjoining  nerve  or  muscle,  adds  confusion  rather  than 
insight.  The  end-plate  potential  falls  much  less  rapidly  than  that  of 
nerve  or  muscle,  and  there  is  no  evidence  of  a  great  store  or  synthesis 
of  ACh  there.  How,  then,  can  the  tremendous  ChE  activity  be  recon- 
ciled with  any  current  theories  relating  ACh  to  neural  functioning? 
When  a  2000  horse-power  engine  is  found  in  a  half-ton  truck,  one  must 
suspect  it  is  there  for  some  other  reason  than  to  supply  ordinary  motive 
power. 

Let  us  agree,  however,  that  ChE  is  not  kept  fully  saturated,  and  con- 
tinue with  these  calculations.  The  cat  cervical  sympathetic  ganglion 
releases  ACh  to  perfusing  fluid,  on  preganglionic  stimulation.  Again 
in  millimoles  per  gram,  the  ACh  content  of  the  ganglion,  before  or 
after  several  hours'  tetanus,  is  about  10"^,  although  five  times  this 
amount  has  been  released  during  the  activity  period.  The  rate  of  re- 
lease falls  with  continued  activity,  but  holds  up  better  when  some  blood 
is  present.  A  maximum  of  10"*  is  liberated  in  five  minutes'  tetanus  at 
17  per  second  or,  per  impulse,  about  2  X  10~^  (cf.  "•  ^^).     The  ACh  es- 


588  ANNALS  NEW  YORK  ACADEMY  OF  SCIENCES 

caping  from  the  ganglion,  per  impulse,  is  thus  only  about  one-fiftieth  of 
the  amount  that  the  ChE  present  could  split  in  one  millisecond.  If  ChA 
activity  is  taken  to  be  one-thousandth  that  of  ChE,  the  ACh  synthesized 
in  60  ms.  (the  interval  between  impulses)  could  equal  that  released  or 
exceed  it  two-  or  three-fold.  This,  incidentally,  leaves  no  place  for  the 
often-assumed  existence  of  a  much  greater  ACh  turnover  within  active 
units  than  is  reflected  in  the  amount  escaping  from  them. 

Isolated  frog  nerve,  according  to  von  Muralt,*^'  actually  increases  its 
ACh  content  on  tetanization,  from  lO"*'  at  rest  to  1.5  X  10"*^  while  ac- 
tive, and  the  ACh  increase  per  impulse  calculates  to  6  X  10"^  from  von 
Muralt's  figures,  to  10~"  from  Lissak's.^^  (In  the  latter  experiments, 
only  the  ACh  diffusing  from  the  cut  ends  of  a  stimulated  nerve  was 
measured.)  For  cat  gastrocnemius,  assuming  a  weight  of  20  grams, 
the  ACh  released  by  a  single  maximal  twitch  evoked  by  the  nerve  is 
6  X  10"^^  mM/gm.^*  Nerve  can,  of  course,  conduct  several  hundred 
impulses  per  second  for  long  periods,  but  we  might  conservatively  cal- 
culate with  50  per  second,  or  20  milliseconds  total  time  available  per 
impulse.  Frog  nerve  ChE  could  split  in  this  period  3  X  10"'^  mM/gm. 
of  ACh:  five  times  the  amount  von  Muralt  finds  liberated  and  30,000 
times  Lissak's  figure.  If,  again,  ChA  is  only  one-thousandth  as  active, 
it  could  easily  supply  ACh  at  the  rate  demanded  by  Lissak  but  would 
fall  short  of  von  Muralt's  figure  by  100-fold.  On  the  basis  of  such 
an  analysis,  a  nerve  should  be  able  to  conduct  an  impulse  only  once 
in  two  seconds.  Von  Muralt's  value,  incidentally,  is  far  more  in  ac- 
cord with  that  for  the  ganglion,  both  in  absolute  amount  and  in  rela- 
tion to  ChE  activity,  and  it  is  also  more  probably  correct  on  method- 
ological grounds.  But  it  cannot  be  right  if  the  assumed  ChA  activity 
is  remotely  correct. 

Perhaps,  then,  all  these  discrepancies  result  from  falsely  low  ChA 
values.  This  enzyme  system  might  easily  have  been  seriously  injured 
during  tissue  extraction  and,  thus,  be  far  more  active  in  vivo.  Let  us 
make  this  assumption,  and  allow  a  ChA  activity  sufficient  to  equal  ChE 
activity  or,  giving  ACh  the  most  favorable  conditions,  an  activity 
sufficient  only  to  cover  the  ACh  actually  released  on  stimulation.  Note, 
however,  that  even  this  excludes  any  greater  ACh  formation  and  sub- 
sequent destruction,  within  the  cell  or  outside  it,  beyond  the  measured 
formation.  If  this  greater  turnover  is  allowed,  by  assuming  ChA 
activity  to  equal  ChE,  the  following  relationships  reveal  still  more  in- 
tolerable discrepancies. 

The  formation  of  one  millimole  of  ACh  requires,  we  have  agreed, 
some  2  calories.     The  sympathetic  ganglion,  releasing  ACh  on  stimula- 


GERARD:   NERVE  METABOLISM   AND  FUNCTION  589 

tion  at  the  rate  of  10~^  mM/gra./hr., would  liberate  2  X  10~^  cal./gm./hr. 
For  frog  nerve,  at  50  impulses  per  second  (and  at  this  frequency  the 
energy  per  impulse  is  fully  80%  of  that  at  zero  frequency) ,  von  Muralt's 
value  gives  10"'  mM  ACh  or  0.02  cal.  (If  ChE  were  working  at  full 
capacity,  the  heat  liberated  just  from  ACh  splitting  would  be  6  cal. 
for  the  ganglion,  0.1  cal.  for  frog  nerve!)  But  the  measured  total  heat 
production  of  frog  nerve  is,  in  these  units,  0.1  cal.  at  rest  and  0.18  at 
maximal  activity;  for  mammalian  cortex  (using  the  highest  values  of 
Q02  reported"") ,  the  resting  energy  release  is  25  cal.  and  that  of  maxi- 
mum activity  perhaps  50  cal.  These  brain  values  are  probably  much  too 
high  for  the  ganglion  (probably  three-fold"^''),  but  this  gives  every  ad- 
vantage to  ACh.  The  actual  ACh  released  in  nerve  during  activity 
would  thus,  duj-ing  its  normal  hydrolysis  by  ChE,  account  for  over  10% 
of  the  total  heat  of  nerve  activity.  Yet,  only  3%  of  this  heat  is  initial 
heat,  immediately  related  to  the  events  of  conduction.  Moreover, 
other  exothermic  reactions  are  surely  involved,  even  with  ACh  itself — 
in  its  formation,  liberation,  neutralization,  etc. — before  that  of  its 
destruction.  (And  again,  if  ChE  were  fully  active,  the  ACh  hydrolysis 
heat  alone  would  account  for  more  than  the  full  extra  heat  production 
of  active  nerve!) 

An  examination,  further,  of  actual  chemical  reactions  involved  in  the 
synthesis  of  ACh  raises  added  difficulties.  The  initial  energy'-  source 
for  ACh  synthesis  is  considered  to  be  CrP.  During  maximal  frog 
nerve  activity,  less  than  13  mgm.  %  of  CrP  is  split  in  an  hour;  enough 
to  account,  at  best,  for  0.007  cal.^",  far  below  the  needs  for  ACh.  But, 
of  course,  CrP  is  resynthesized  by  energy  from  other  metabolic  reac- 
tions, so  this  does  not  mean  too  much.  The  total  fuel  turnover,  how- 
ever, does  set  an  inescapable  limit.  For  bullfrog  nerve,  6  mgm.  %  of 
carbohydrate  disappears  per  gram  per  hour  at  rest  or  activity ;''®  for 
the  small  frog  nerve,  this  might  be  10  mgm.  %,  or  6  X  10~^  mM/gm./hr. 
On  complete  oxidation,  this  could  yield  a  maximum  of  0.02  mM  of  CrP, 
if  all  the  energy  available  to  form  high-energ\'  phosphate  bands  (3  per 
atom  of  oxygen)  were  so  directed.  Thus,  the  total  nerve  metabolism 
could  just  comfortably  synthesize  ACh  at  the  rate  it  is  reported  actually 
to  form  during  activity  (.01  mM  ACh  from  .02  CrP),  and  could  not 
begin  to  supply  energy  to  synthesize  it  at  the  rate  ChE  can  destroy  it. 
(Actually,  the  picture  is  worse  than  here  presented,  because  the  maxi- 
mum heat  of  activity  is  0.18  cal./gm./hr.  for  frog  nerve,  whereas  the 
assumed  carbohydrate  oxidation  would  yield  0.4.) 

It  may  also  deserve  thought  that,  while  the  esterase  is  located  in  the 
membrane  of  the  giant  nerve  fiber,  the  oxidizing  enzyme  systems  are 


590  ANNALS  NEW  YORK  ACADEMY  OF  SCIENCES 

distributed  through  its  volume.  It  seems  impossible  that  any  con- 
siderable fraction  of  the  oxidative  energy  released  in  the  core  of  such 
a  fiber  could  be  utilized  to  drive  reactions  in  its  surface,  up  to  400 
micra  distant;  and  it  seems  unlikely  even  in  the  usual  medullated  fibers, 
up  to  20  micra  in  diameter.  Along  the  same  lines,  I  know  of  no  evi- 
dence for  the  intensive  respiration  at  neuro-myal  junctions  which 
would  be  demanded  to  keep  up  with  the  terrific  ChE  activity. 
There  is  definite  evidence  against  an  intense  respiration  at  syn- 
aptic regions  in  the  central  nervous  system,  despite  their  greater 
relative  surfaces  for  fiber  propagation  and  junctional  transmission.'^'' 
(A  high  ChE  and  DPN  concentration  in  the  synaptic  layers  of  the 
retina,  however,  has  recently  been  reported.") 

A  final  calculation,  dealing  with  materials  rather  than  energetics,  is 
only  suggestive.  Most  workers  have  tacitly  or  explicitly  assumed  that 
the  acetate  formed  by  hydrolysis  of  ACh  was  not  re-utilized  for  ACh 
synthesis.  Indeed,  ACh  has  been  found  by  Lipton"^  to  form  only  from 
pyruvate  in  oxygen  or  an  acetate  source  (acetoacetic  or  citric  acids) 
in  nitrogen,  under  present  in  vitro  conditions.  This  would  exclude 
full  re-utilization  of  acetate,  even  in  nitrogen,  unless  the  reaction,  2 
acetate  -^  acetoacetate,  is  fully  reversible.  Lipmann  has  just  indicated 
that  synthesis  from  acetate  may  be  possible  when  additional  com- 
ponents, of  coenzyme  character,  are  added,  and  this  would  make  easier 
a  cyclic  use  of  acetate.  Without  cyclic  use,  a  molecule  of  glucose  would 
have  to  be  lost  for  every  two  of  ACh  formed  and  hydrolyzed — or  twenty- 
fold  the  actual  rate  in  nerve,  0.01  mM  ACh;  0.0006  glucose.  The 
accompanying  heat  production  would  have  to  be  similarly  outrageous, 
in  comparison  with  the  factual  rate. 

Such  quantitative  considerations  are  admittedly  rough,  with  little 
attention  to  detailed  conditions  (temperature,  species,  rate  of  stimula- 
tion, etc.),  but  the  order  of  magnitude  cannot  be  far  off.  They  demon- 
strate conclusively,  I  believe,  that  ChE  cannot  possibly  exert  its  full 
activity  on  ACh  in  neural  tissues  and  suggest  that  other  meanings  for 
its  presence  and  action  be  sought.  Further,  even  the  less  drastic  rates 
and  amounts  reported  for  other  phases  of  an  ACh  system  lead  to  severe 
quantitative  strains  on  the  total  metabolism  of  nerve  or  brain.  But 
still  other  difficulties  have  been  brought  out  in  this  publication. 

Drug  Action 

Drugs,  especially  esterase  inhibitors,  have  been  widely  used  in  study- 
ing the  ACh  system,  and  their  actions  have  been  much  discussed  dur- 
ing this  symposium.     The  point  of  greatest  debate  has  been  the  ques- 


GERARD:   NERVE  METABOLISM  AND  FUNCTION  591 

tion  of  permeability;  for,  of  course,  the  absence  of  an  expected  effect 
in  vivo  could  easily  be  due  to  a  failure  of  the  added  substance  to  pene- 
trate to  the  vulnerable  region.  However,  the  evidence  marshalled 
seems  to  be  conclusive  that  esterase  can  be  inactivated,  or  ACh  con- 
tent increased,  without  serious  disturbance  of  function  of  nerve  or 
muscle. 

All  agree  that  eserine,  a  tertiary  amine,  can  enter  nerve  and  muscle, 
and  Nachmansohn  makes  the  point  that  the  action  potential  of  squid 
nerve  can  be  abohshed  by  soaking  in  this  drug.  (The  fall  of  the  ac- 
tion potential  does  not  show  the  great  prolongation  one  might  expect  if 
ACh  removal  were  interfered  with.)  Yet  Cantoni  and  Loewi  have  re- 
ported"^ that  a  frog  can  be  eserinized  in  vivo  so  that  nerve  ChE  activity 
is  abolished,  while  nerve  conduction  remains  undisturbed.  (Con- 
versely, intravenous  ChE  blocks  the  pupillary  reflex  in  rats.*°)  A 
comparable  result  with  the  even  more  powerful,  and  irreversible,  in- 
hibitor, diisopropyl-fluorophosphate,  has  just  been  presented  by  Oilman 
and  by  Bodansky.  Both  in  vivo  and  in  vitro,  though  with  some 
anomalies  in  behavior,  this  agent  has  been  shown  to  inactivate  entirely 
ChE  while  leaving  nerve  conduction  and  action  potentials  intact.  Al- 
though detailed  criticisms  have  been  made,  especially  by  Talbot,  the 
major  fact  remains,  as  in  the  eserine  experiments,  that  conduction  with- 
out esterase  is  possible.*  Again,  veratrm  can  inhibit  ChE,^^  yet  it  does 
not^-  influence  muscle  or  the  neuromyal  junction,  including  its  sensitiv- 
ity to  added  ACh,  except  for  a  late  and  independent  negativity. 

The  inability  of  ACh,  added  in  large  concentration  to  the  surround- 
ing medium,  to  depolarize  nerve  or  otherwise  to  disturb  conduction,  has 
been  reemphasized  by  the  new  experiments  of  Lorente  de  No  and  of 
Bronk.  Nachmansohn  has  urged  that  ACh,  a  quarternary  ion,  cannot 
penetrate  the  lipoid  membrane  of  nerve  fibers,  except  at  their  naked 
terminals,  thus  accounting  for  these  negative  results.  Yet  ACh  does 
leave  nerve  trunks  on  stimulation  and  should,  similarly,  be  able  to 
enter  under  combined  anoxia  and  stimulation.  Further,  both  Bronk 
and  Atcheson  have  presented  clear  evidence  that  tetraethylammonium 


*  The  results  of  Gilman  and  the  Edgewood  workers  have  since  been  challenged  by  Nachman- 
sohn and  his  colleagues.  Both  groups  reported  work  at  the  April  meeting  of  the  Federation,  and 
their  full  papers  have  since  appeared  (J.  Neurophysiol.  June).  Work  done  in  the  interval  in  my 
laboratory  fully  supports  the  conclusions  of  the  Edgewood  group. 

Frog  sciatics  were  immersed  in  peanut  oil,  with  or  without  DFP,  resting  on  stimulating  and 
lead-off  electrodes.  Action  potentials  fail  in  a  few  minutes  or  remain  normal  for  hours,  depending 
on  the  drug  concentration.  Conduction,  when  lost,  is  not  restored  in  fresh  oil.  A  nerve  exposed 
for  an  hour  to  a  non-depressing  concentration  of  DFP,  washed,  ground,  and  assayed  for  cholin- 
esterase  by  its  rate  of  destruction  of  added  acetylcholine  (tested  on  the  frog's  rectus),  shows  no 
cholinesterase  activity.  A  companion  nerve  continues  to  conduct  well,  while  remaining  in  the  same 
DFP  solution.  Further,  when  a  washed,  poisoned  nerve  is  ground  together  with  an  untreated  one, 
the  homogenate  assays  at  the  average  cholinesterase  activity  of  the  two  nerves  taken  separately. 
The  DFP  inactivation  of  cholinesterase  occurs,  therefore,  prior  to  the  grinding.  Clearly,  conduc- 
tion is  possible  in  nerve  lacking  cholinesterase. 


592  ANNALS  NEW  YORK  ACADEMY  OF  SCIENCES 

chloride,  another  quarternary,  acts  powerfully  on  medullated  nerve 
and  so,  presumably,  penetrates  easily. 

A  seemingly  crucial  test  has  been  carried  out  in  the  last  few  weeks 
in  our  laboratory  by  Miss  Graham.  ACh  Br  (1 :  1000  in  isotonic  KCl 
with  vital  red)  was  injected  into  single  muscle  fibers  of  the  eserinized 
frog  sartorius  with  a  micropipette,  and  the  membrane  potential  meas- 
ured. With  one  electrode  inside  the  fiber  and  another  outside,  mem- 
brane potentials  of  40  to  80  mV  are  regularly  obtained.  Injection  of 
a  large  drop  of  isotonic  KCl  with  or  without  ACh,  large  enough  to  fill 
the  fiber  cross-section  and  spread  one  to  three  diameters  along  its 
length,  will  immediately  lower  the  potential  by  one-  to  two-thirds;  but 
a  smaller  drop,  not  filling  the  entire  cross-section,  has  little  effect;  only 
7%  fall,  in  one  fully  satisfactory  experiment  with  ACh.  It  seems,  then, 
that  ACh  inside  the  membrane  does  not  depolarize  it,  as  postulated 
by  Nachmansohn  and  by  Beutner  and  Barnes. 

A  number  of  other  points  have  been  raised  here,  which  must  also  be 
kept  in  mind.  Bodansky  has  emphasized  the  existence  of  a  family  of 
esterases,  even  in  different  neural  structures  of  the  same  species:  e.g., 
the  enzymes  in  cervical  sympathetic  ganglion  and  in  brain  show  different 
substrate  and  ion  concentration  optima,  different  equations  relating 
concentration  to  activity,  different  substrate  selectivities,  etc.  The 
last  point  is  especially  important,  since  a  criterion  urged  for  discrim- 
inating between  "true"  and  "pseudo"  esterase  is  the  relative  inactivity 
of  the  true  enzyme  with  tributyrin.  Yet  the  "true"  esterase  of  brain 
splits  triacetin  up  to  six  times  faster  than  it  does  ACh. 

The  distribution  of  ChE  or  ACh,  or  both,  in  various  organs  and  tis- 
sues has  also  been  mentioned  by  several  discussants.  Rosenblueth 
asked  about  conduction  in  adrenergic  nerve  fibers,  which  lack  the  ACh 
system ;  Hoagland  made  a  similar  point  about  Nitella,  which  conducts 
independently  of  ACh;  another  discussant  mentioned  a  recent  report 
that  ChE  is  absent  in  the  electric  organ  of  Malaptorurus ;  and  the  caro- 
tid body,  although  specifically  sensitive  to  ACh,  is  reported  to  lack 
ChE.^^  ChE  is  also  absent  from  the  iris  sphincter  of  the  amphibian 
eye,  while  present  in  its  cornea  and  in  the  turtle's  sphincter.^*  Con- 
versely, parts  of  the  ACh  system  are  richly  present  in  spleen,  placenta, 
cornea,  potatoes,  and  some  bacteria,  where  any  relation  to  neural  func- 
tion is  nearly,  or  quite,  impossible.  And  finally,  in  this  connection, 
many  other  agents  act  on,  and  other  enzymes  are  present  in,  neurones. 
Adrenalin  keeps  up  the  action  potential  in  isolated  cat  nerve  ;^^  ATP 
stimulates  smooth,  as  well  as  striped,  muscle  ;^^  carbonic  anhydrase  is 
interestingly  distributed  in  the  brain  ;^^  CO2  has  marked  and  differential 


GERARD:   NERVE  METABOLISM  AND  FUNCTION  593 

actions  on  the  nervous  system;  and  so  on.  Thiamin  is  reported^^"^^ 
to  affect  ACh  action  and  synthesis  and  to  be  hberated  from  pre- 
cursor in  relatively  large  amounts  from  stimulated  frog  nerve  even  to 
be  the  transmitter.  Cocarboxylase,  like  ChE,  is  concentrated  in  the 
nerve  membrane.®-  (For  discussion  of  further  recent  evidence,  see 
Gerard  and  Libet.")  I  do  not  see  how  we  can  reasonably  select  the 
ACh  system  from  all  this  welter  and  just  assign  to  it  an  essential  role 
in  conduction  of  the  nerve  impulse. 

THE  PROBLEM  OF  JUNCTIONAL  TRANSMISSION 

In  the  time  available,  the  problem  of  junctional  transmission,  pre- 
sented mainly  by  Eccles,  can  only  be  touched  upon,  and  even  so  the 
case  of  autonomic  effectors,  the  classical  one  of  neurohumoral  action, 
which  has  not  been  before  us,  will  be  omitted.  As  for  the  neuro-myal 
junction,  the  unquestioned  facts,  that  ChE  is  more  concentrated  there 
than  elsewhere  in  the  muscle  fiber  by  a  factor  of  10*  (Nachmansohn), 
and  that  this  region  is  more  sensitive  to  added  ACh  by  a  similar  factor 
(Kuffler),  are  impressive;  along  with  the  potentiating  and  prolonging 
action  of  eserine,  long  known  for  junction  as  well  as  ganglion.  I  am, 
personally,  less  convinced  of  a  transmitter  role  of  ACh  at  the  junction 
than  I  was  a  few  years  back,  but  do  not  consider  that  the  evidence  is 
crucial  in  either  direction.  (The  observation®^  that  the  lizard  muscle 
fiber  can  respond  to  nerve  stimulation  at  a  time  when  ACh  applied 
to  the  end-plate  is  ineffective,  although  such  ACh  does  cause  contrac- 
tion when  first  administered,  has  not  been  explained.  Also,  the  end- 
plate  potential,  often  supposed  to  be  set  up  by  ACh  liberation,  is  pres- 
ent in  invertebrate,  as  in  vertebrate,  muscle;  but  the  end-plate  region 
in  the  former  is  not  sensitive  to  ACh  or  to  curare.®*)  Discussion  here 
has  been  mostly  on  central  synapses,  and  the  reader  should  consider 
these. 

As  elaborated  by  Eccles,  the  currents  that  flow  between  an  active 
fiber  region  and  an  inactive  one,  whether  in  the  same  or  another  unit, 
do  account  for  the  usual  activation  phenomena.  The  results  of  Ar- 
vanitaki-*  and  of  many  other  recent  experimenters®^'  ®®'  ®^  show  that 
threshold  changes  and  transmission  from  unit  to  unit  in  simple  sys- 
tems are  accurately  and  quantitatively  explicable  in  terms  of  the  meas- 
ured currents  and  the  known  geometry.  Whether  Eccles'  detailed  anal- 
ysis of  the  situation  at  a  synapse  will  hold  up  as  well  with  time  as  he 
was  able  to  defend  it  here,  we  do  not  know,  but  there  is  every  reason 
to  push  such  thinking  further.  (Some  difficulties  are:  the  very  variable 
structures  which  are  found  in  synapses,  where  the  two  units  may  meet  as 


594  ANNALS  NEW  YORK  ACADEMY  OF  SCIENCES 

parallel  fibers,  spirals  of  one  on  another,  multiple  contacts,  etc.,  as  well 
as  the  orthodox  small  end-foot  stuck  on  a  large  surface,  like  a  match 
stick  on  a  cheese;  the  problem  of  multiple  synapses  on  a  cell  body  and 
the  relative  area  of  end-feet  and  their  surround;  the  presence  of  ir- 
reciprocal conduction  with  protoplasmic  continuity,  as  in  an  asymmet- 
rically compressed  sartorius  muscle.  Neither  these,  nor  the  spatial 
theory  of  inhibition,  nor  the  need  for  regarding  the  E.  E.  G.  as  an  oscil- 
lating somatic  potential,  problems  which  have  received  attention  in 
this  publication  and  elsewhere,^*  can  be  here  expanded.) 

The  evidence  for  a  transmitter  role  of  ACh  in  the  central  nervous 
system,  on  the  contrary,  is  inferential  and  conflicting.  Those  who 
have  read  Feldberg's  recent  review  of  this  question'^  must  have  been 
impressed  by  the  poor  case  that  can  be  made.  Added  eserine,  or  ACh, 
or  both,  may  increase  the  activity  of  a  brain  region,  or  depress  it,  or 
cause  negligible  change.  The  failure  of  ACh  to  alter  frog  cord  re- 
flexes, mentioned  by  Eccles,  is  a  case  in  point.  Or  ACh  may  excite, 
while  eserine  depresses.  Atropine,  on  the  whole,  does  nothing.  The 
two  compounds  mentioned  by  Oilman,  both  powerful  anti-esterases  and 
both  able  to  produce  convulsions,  one  of  which  is  completely  antidoted 
by  atropine,  while  the  other  is  uninfluenced  by  it,  afford  an  instance 
of  the  conflicting  facts  in  this  area.  Strychnine  is  supposed  to  exert  its 
action  by  blocking  ChE,  yet  Tobias^^  has  found  the  ACh  content  of 
frog  and  rat  brains  and  cords  decreased,  if  anything,  by  strychnine. 
Nembutal,  conversely,  increases  the  ACh  content,  although,  as  Bo- 
dansky  mentioned,  it  also  lowers  ChE  activity.  If  ACh  is  an  agent 
for  evoking  neurone  activity,  it  should  increase  the  oxygen  consump- 
tion of  brain.  Lipton  has  recently  made  Q02  measurements  on  rat 
brain  slices,  at  my  request,  and  found  no  influence  of  eserine  (10"^)  alone 
with  eserine  and  ACh  (10"^),  at  most  a  better  maintenance  of  the  usual 
initial  rate.  Incidentally,  the  only  other  observations  I  have  found  on 
the  influence  of  ACh  on  respiration  are  one"°  showing  an  increase  in 
salivary  gland  oxygen  consumption,  and  a  forgotten  one  from  my  own 
laboratory^^^  showing  a  marked  decrease  in  the  oxygen  consumption 
of  nerve  (uneserinized). 

The  best  basis  for  invoking  chemicals  in  synaptic  transmission  is 
that  synaptic  potentials,  like  those  of  the  end-plate,  may  last  much 
longer  than  could  any  reasonable  physical  discharge  period  for  mem- 
branes with  capacitances  and  resistances  in  the  range  known.  Then  one 
invokes  some  active  depolarization  process,  as  for  nerve;  and  then  this 
must  be  explained,  by  a  chemical  reaction  of  some  sort.  To  be  sure, 
chemical  activity  is  involved,  as  in  nerve,  and  quantitatively  more  in- 


GERARD:   NERVE  METABOLISM  AND  FUNCTION  595 

tense.  But  is  the  chemical  ACh?  Quien  sabef  It  should  be  recalled 
that  undrugged  nerve  also  has  an  enduring  after-potential,  which  can 
increase  in  intensity  for  several  minutes  and  persist  for  ten  or  more. 


40 


CONCLUSION 

Dr.  Nachmansohn  skillfully  and  generously  organized  the  extraor- 
dinarily successful  conference  of  which  this  is  the  result,  to  bring  forth 
much  current  evidence  and  a  full  range  of  judgments  bearing  on  the 
significance  of  ACh,  as  well  as  of  electrical  changes,  for  the  functioning 
of  nerve  and  other  tissues.  With  these  facts  and  arguments  before  us, 
we  must  conclude  that  ACh  is  not  critically  involved  in  nerve  conduc- 
tion, and  we  must  be  reserved  in  assigning  it  a  role  in  junctional  trans- 
mission, particularly  within  the  nervous  system.  This  is  progress  and 
should  lead  to  greater  progress.  Our  thinking  and  our  consequent  ex- 
perimentation now  can  be  directed  along  new  lines. 

This  is  not  to  say  that  the  hypotheses  which  must  be  relinquished 
have  been  worthless,  nor  that  the  ACh  system  is  unimportant.  Hy- 
potheses are  not  true  or  false  (who  can  assert  absolute  truth?) ;  they 
are  useful  or  useless.  They  do  or  do  not  suggest  investigations  which 
reveal  new  facts,  facts  which  discriminate  between  alternate  views  or 
which  fill  in  gaps  of  felt  ignorance  or  which  suggest  new  interpretations 
and  experiments.  By  such  standards,  the  various  ACh  hypotheses  have 
been  good ;  they  have  been  abundantly  fruitful.  But  this  fruit  is  ripe, 
and  it  is  time  for  the  seed  of  a  new  idea  to  be  germinated.  Fresh  fruit 
will  then  ripen  with  time  and  the  present  crop  not  be  husbanded  until 
it  rots  or  dries  up. 

What  a  new  and  fertile  approach  may  be,  I  do  not  know.  ACh  and 
the  enzymes  that  operate  in  the  system  can  hardly  be  present  adven- 
titiously. Nature  no  more  evolved  the  ACh  system  to  mislead  bio- 
chemists than  it  evolved  the  giant  nerve  fiber  to  aid  physiologists.  ACh 
has  some  significance  to  cells.  Perhaps  this  system  is  a  fragment  of  a 
universally  important  metabolic  mechanism,  dealing,  if  one  must  hazard 
a  particular  guess,  with  the  manipulation  of  lipid  molecules.  Such  facts 
or  statements  as  the  following  maj'-  serve  as  clues.  ACh  prevents  the 
splitting  of  CrP  by  muscle  juice  ;^°-  choline  lack  increases  the  turnover 
of  phosphohpids;^°^  ACh  can  replace  Ca  in  enabhng  myosin  to  split 
^'pp.52  ^Qi^  jg  |.j-jg  Qj^jy  system  able  to  capture  energy  via  both  respira- 
tory and  glycolytic  reactions,^^  and  so  is  related  to  both  respiration 
and  carbohydrate  utilization  rates. ^°*'  "^  It  would  still  be  possible  for 
evolution  to  have  selected  this  fragment  of  a  more  general  system  for 
special  emphasis  and  functioning  in  particular  situations;  to  serve,  for 


596  ^A^A^.4L^  NEW  YORK  ACADEMY  OF  SCIENCES 

example,  as  a  transmitter  at  parasympathetic  endings.  After  all,  ACh 
is  an  ion  with  rather  striking  physico-chemical  properties.  In  just 
such  fashion  have  the  ubiquitous  respiratory  hemins  of  cells  been  se- 
lected for  the  special  function  of  transporting  oxygen.  The  parallel 
evolution  of  hemoglobin  in  utterly  separate  phyla,  as  the  vertebrates 
and  annelids,  was  a  great  mystery  before  the  discovery  that  such  re- 
lated molecules  as  cytochrome  are  almost  universally  present  in  cells. 
I  am  suggesting,  then,  that  ACh  may  extend  further  and  have  more 
importance  in  cell  functioning  than  has  yet  been  seriously  considered 
and  that  any  particular  role  it  plays  in  transmission  is  a  secondary  and 
derivative  one.  In  arguing,  as  I  have,  for  renouncing  the  belief  that 
ACh  has  any  direct  function  in  nerve  conduction  and  in  transmission 
at  many  junctions,  I  am  inviting  those  who  work  with  the  ACh  sys- 
tem to  emerge  from  the  chrysalis  which  they  have  outgrown  and  to  seek 
fresher  and  greater  fields  of  intellectual  nourishment. 

BIBLIOGRAPHY 

1.  Gerard,  R.  W. 

1927.     Science.  66:  495. 

2.  Gerard,  R.  W. 

1931.  Quart.  Rev.  Biol.  6:  59. 

3.  Gerard,  R.  W. 

1932.  Physiol.  Rev.  12:  469. 

4.  Fillie,  H. 

1908.     Zeitsch.  f.  d.  AUgem.  Physiol.  8:  492. 

5.  Feng,  T.  P. 

1936.  Ergeb.  Physiol.  38:  73. 

6.  Wilhelmi,  A.  E. 

1946.     In:  Fulton,  Howell's  Textbook  of  Physiology.    See  p.  56.     Saunders. 
Philadelphia. 

7.  Drury,  A.  N. 

1932.     J.  Physiol.  74:  147. 

8.  Green,  H.  N.,  &  H.  B.  Storer 
1944.     Brit.  J.  Exp.  Path.  25:  150. 

9.  Buchtal,  F.,  A.  Deutsch,  &  G.  G.  Knappsrs 
1944.     Nature.  153:  774. 

10.  Gerard,  R.  W.,  &  N.  Tupikova 

1938.  J.  Cell.  Comp.  Physiol.  12:  325. 

11.  Quastel,  J.  H. 

1939.  Physiol.  Rev.  19:  135.  "^ 

12.  Garard,  R.  W. 

1937.  Ann.  Rev.  Biochem.  6:  419. 

13.  Gerard,  R.  W. 

1938.  Arch.  Neurol.  &  Psych.  (Chicago).  40:  985. 

14.  Gerard,  R.  W.,  &  B.  Libet 

.1946.     Progress  in  Neuropsychology  and  Psychiatry.     New  York. 

15.  Hyden,  H. 

1943.     Acta  Physiol.  Scand.  6:  Suppl.  17. 


GERARD:   NERVE  METABOLISM  AND  FUNCTION  597 

16.  Gerard,  R.  W. 

1930.     J.  Physiol.  92:498. 

17.  Shanes,  A.  M.,  &  D.  E.  S.  Brown 

1942.     .1.  Oil.  C;omp.  Physiol.  19:  I. 

18.  Ronzoni,  £. 

19.31.     .1.  Biol.  Chem.  92:  iii. 

19.  Chang,  T.  H.,  &  R.  W.  Gerard 

1933.  J.  Physiol.  104:  291. 

20.  Gerard,  R.  W. 

1934.  Science.  79:  Suppl.  20. 

21.  Curtis,  H.  J.,  &  K.  S.  Cole 

1940.     .J.  CxiW.  Comp.  Physiol.  15:  147. 

22.  Hodgekin,  A.  L.,  &  A.  F.  Huxley 

194.5.     .1.  Phy,siol.  104:  17(1 

23.  Graham,  J.,  G.  R.  Carlson,  &  R.  W.  Gerard 

1942.     Fed.  Proc.  1:29. 

24.  Arvanitaki,  A.  J. 

1942.     .J.  Neurophys.  5:  89. 
26.  Hodgekin,  A.  L. 

1938.  Proc.  Hoy.  Soc.  Loridou  H  126:  87. 

26.  Katz,  B. 

1939.  Electrical  Excitation  of  Xerve.     Oxford  Univ.  Press.     London. 

27.  Libet,  B.,  &  R.  W.  Gerard 
1939.     J.  Neurophys.  2:  1.53. 

28.  CoUe,  J. 

1934.     Compt.  rend.  Soc.  hiol.  117:  99. 

29.  Gerard,  R.  W. 

1934.     J.  Physiol.  83:24. 

30.  Gerard,  R.  W. 

193fi.     Cold  Spring  Harbor  Symposia.  8:  194. 

31.  Tasaki,  I. 

1939.     .J.  Physiol.  127:211. 

32.  Hodgekin,  A.  L. 

1937.     .1.  Physiol.  90:  183,  211. 

33.  Blair,  E.  A.,  &  J.  Erlanger 
1939.     .J.  Physiol.  126:97. 

34.  Lillie,  R.  S. 

1925.     .1.  Gen.  Physiol.  7:  473. 

35.  Marshall,  W.  H.,  &  R.  W.  Gerard 
1933.     .J.  Physiol.  104:. 58(J. 

36.  Hursh,  J.  B. 

1939.     .J.  Physiol.  127:  131. 

37.  Gasser,  H.  S. 

1937.     In:  Gasser  &  Erlanger,  Electrical  Signs  of  Nervous  Activity.     Univ.  of 
Pennsylvania  Press.     Philadelphia. 

38.  Gerard,  R.  W. 

1927.     J.  Physiol.  82:381. 

39.  Hill,  A.  V. 

1933.     Proc.  Roy.  Soc.  London  B  113:  34.5. 

40.  Gerard,  R.  W. 

1930.     .J.  Physiol.  93:337. 

41.  Gerard,  R.  W.,  &  W.  H.  Marshall 
1933.     J.  Physiol.  104:. 53.5. 

42.  Kuffler,  S.  W. 

1946.     Ann.  N.  Y.  Acad.  Sci.  47  (.5).  (In  Press.) 


598  ANNALS  NEW  YORK  ACADEMY  OF  SCIENCES 

43.  Kharasch,  M.  R.,  R.  R.  Legault,  A.  B.  Wilder,  &  R.  W.  Gerard 

1936.     J.  Biol.  Cheni.  113:  537,  557. 

44.  Colowick,  S.  P.,  &  E.  W.  Sutherland 

1942.     J.  Biol.  C'hem.  144:  423. 

45.  Warburg,  O.,  &  W.  Christian 
1942.     Biochem.  Z.  310:384. 

46.  Boyer,  P.  D.,  H.  A.  Lardy,  &  P.  H.  Phillips 
1942.     .J.  Biol.  Chem.  146:  675. 

47.  Bailey,  K. 

1942.     Biochem.  J.  36:  121. 

48.  Engelhardt,  W.  A.,  &  M.  N.  Lyubimova 

1942.  Biochimia.  7:  205. 

49.  DuBois,  K.  P.,  H.  G.  Albaum,  &  V.  R.  Potter 

1943.  .1.  B'nA.  C'hem.  147:  699. 

50.  Greville,  G.  D.,  &  H.  Schwann 
1943.     Nature.  152:  81. 

51.  Utter,  M.  F.,  &  C.  H.  Workman 

1942.  J.  Biol.  Chem.  146:289. 

52.  DuBois,  K.  P.,  &  V.  R.  Potter 

1943.  J.  Biol.  Chem.  148:  451. 

53.  DuBois,  K.  P.,  &  V.  R.  Potter 

1943.  J.  Biol.  Chem.  150:  185. 

54.  Lipmann,  F.,  &  L.  J.  Tuttle 

1944.  J.  Biol.  Chem.  153:  571. 

55.  Feldberg,  W.,  &  T.  Mann 

1945.  J.  Physiol.  104:  8. 

56.  Lardy,  H.  A.,  &  C.  A.  Elvehjem 
1945.     Ann.  Rev.  Biochem.  14:  1. 

57.  Evans,  E.  A, 

1944.  Ami.  Rev.  Biochem.  13:  187. 

58.  Szent-Gyorgyi,  A. 

1945.  Acta  Physiol.  Scand.  9:  Suppl.  25. 

59.  Hegenauer,  A.  H.,  W.  O.  Fenn,  &  D.  M.  Cobb 

1934.     J.  Cell.  Comp.  Physiol.  4:  .505. 

60.  Tipton,  S.  R. 

1936.     J.  Cell.  Comp.  Physiol.  7:  433. 

61.  Gerard,  R.  W.,  &  N.  Tupikova 

1939.     J.  Cell.  Comp.  Phy.siol.  13:  1. 

62.  Boyer,  P.  D.,  H.  A,  Lardy,  &  P.  H.  Phillips 

1943.     J.  Biol.  Chem.  149:  .529. 

63.  Schallek,  W. 

1945.     J.  Cell.  Comp.  Physiol.  26:  15. 

64.  Montigel,  C. 

1943.     Helv.  Physiol.  Pharm.  Acta.  1:  c.  47. 

65.  Dubner,  H.  N.,  &  R.  W.  Gerard 

1939.  J.  Neurophysiol.  2:  142. 

66.  Fenn,  W.  O. 

1940.  Physiol.  Rev.  20:  377. 

67.  von  Muralt,  A. 

1942.     PHlig.  Arch.  245:  604. 

68.  Lissak,  K. 

1939.     J.  Physiol.  127:263. 

69.  Nachmansohn,  D. 

1945.     Vitamins  &  Hormones.  3:  337. 

70.  Pearce,  J.,  &  R.  W.  Gerard 

1942.     .1.  Physiol.  136:49. 


GERARD:    NERVE   METABOLISM   AND   FUNCTION  599 

71.  Feldberg,  W. 

1945.     Physiol.  Rev.  26:  .596. 

72.  Brown,  G.  L.,  &  W.  Feldberg 
1936.     .J.  Physiol.  88:26.5. 

73.  Mackintosh,  F.  C. 
1938.     J.  Physiol.  94:  155. 

74.  Brown,  G.  L.,  H.  H.  Dale,  &  W.  Feldberg 

1936.  J.  Physiol.  87:394. 

75.  Gerard,  R.  W. 

1937.  Proc.  Assoc.  Res.  Nerv.  &  Ment.  Dis.  18:  316. 

76.  Holmes,  E.  G.,  R.  W.  Gerard,  &  E.  I.  Solomon 
1930.     .J.  Physiol.  93:342. 

76a.  Tobias,  J.  M.,  D.  B.  Clark,  &  R.  W.  Gerard 

1942.  Fed.  Proc.  1:85. 

77.  Anfinsen,  C.  B. 

1944.     J.  Biol.  Chem.  152:  267,  285. 

78.  Lipton,  M.  A. 
Unpublished  data. 

79.  Cantoni,  G.  L.,  &  O.  Loewi 

1944.  J.  Pharm.  Exp.  Therap.  81:  67. 

80.  Mendel,  B.,  &  R.  D.  Hawkins 

1943.  J.  Neurophysiol.  6:  431. 

81.  Nachmansohn,  D.,  &  H.  Schnermann 

1945.  J.  Biol.  Chem.  159:  239. 

82.  Kuffler,  S.  W. 

1945.     .J.  Neurophysiol.  8:  113. 

83.  Hollinshead,  W.  B.,  &  C.  H.  Sawyer 
1945.     Anat.  Rec.  91:  282. 

84.  Armstrong,  P.  B. 

1943.  J.  Cell.  Comp.  Physiol.  22:  1. 

85.  Bulbring,  E.,  &  D.  Whitteridge 

1941.  J.  Physiol.  99:201. 

86.  Buchtal,  F.,  &  G.  Kahlson 

1944.  Acta  Physiol.  Scand.  8:  317,  325. 

87.  Ashby,  W. 

1944.  J.  Biol.  Chem.  156:  323.  331. 

88.  von  Muralt,  A. 

1945.  E.xperientia.  1(5):  1. 

89.  Torda,  C,  H.  Wolff 

1944.     Proc.  Soc.  Exp.  Biol.  &  Med.  53:  88. 

90.  Liechti,  A.,  A.  von  Muralt,  &  M.  Reinert 

1943.  Helv.  Physiol.  Acta.  1:  79. 

91.  Wyss,  F. 

1944.  Helv.  Physiol.  Acta.  2:  121. 

92.  Nachmansohn,  D.,  &  H.  B.  Steinbach 

1942.  ,J.  Neurophysiol.  5:  109. 

93.  Buchtal,  F.,  &  J.  Lindhard 
1937.     J.  Physiol.  90:82. 

94.  Katz,  B.,  &  S.  W.  Kuffler 
Personal  Communication. 

95.  Marrazzi,  A.  S.,  &  R.  Lorente  de  No 
1944.     J.  Neurophysiol.  7:  83. 

96.  Rosenblueth,  A. 

1944.  J.  Physiol.  141 :  196. 

97.  Skoglund,  C.  R. 

1945.  J.  Neurophysiol.  8:  365.  377. 


600  ANNALS  NEW  YORK  ACADEMY  OF  SCIENCES 

98.  Gerard,  R.  W. 

1941.     Ohio  J.  Sci.  41:  160. 

99.  Tobias,  J.  M.,  M.  A.  Lipton,  &  A.  Lepinat 

1946.     Proc.  Soc.  Exp.  Biol.  &  Med.  61:  51. 

100.  Brock,  N.,  H.  Druckrey,  &  W.  Loch 

1943.  Biochem.  Z.  313 :  300. 

101.  Shaffer,  M.,  T.  H.  Chang,  &  R.  W.  Gerard 
193.^).     J.  Physiol.  111:697. 

102.  Abdon,  N.  O.,  &  N.  A.  Nielsen 
1937.     Skand.  Arch.  Physiol.  77:  13. 

103.  Patterson,  J.  M.,  N.  B.  Keevil,  &  E,  W.  McHenry 

1944.  J.  Biol.  Chem.  153:  489. 

104.  Welch,  J.  H.,  &  J.  E.  Hyde 

1944.     J.  Neurophysiol.  7:41. 

106.  Sanz,  M.  C. 

1943.     Pflus.  Arch.  246:  596. 


CONCLUSION 

Remarks  Made  at  a  Dinner  in  the  Hotel  Astor,  New  York,  after  the 

Conference   on    The   Mechanism   of   Nerve   Activity,    Sponsored   by 

The  New  York  Academy  of  Sciences,  February  9,  1946. 

By  J.  F.  Fulton 

Sterling  Frojessor  of  Physiology,  Yale  University  School  of  Medicine, 

New  Haven,  Connecticut 

No  set  speeches  have  been  planned  for  this  evening,  since  those  who 
arranged  the  Conference  wished  to  keep  our  proceedings  entirely  in- 
formal. The  Committee  has  asked  me  to  express  our  most  sincere 
thanks  to  The  New  York  Academy  of  Sciences  and,  particularly,  to  Mrs. 
Miner  and  her  gracious  assistants,  who  have  done  so  much,  both  before 
and  during  the  Conference,  to  make  it  outstandingly  successful. 

In  this  connection,  I  must  also  mention  the  man — and  I  do  this 
without  instructions  from  the  Committee — who  originally  conceived 
the  idea  of  having  the  Conference  and  who,  with  Tracy  Putnam's  ener- 
getic backing,  has  been  so  largely  responsible  for  working  out  the  de- 
tails. David  Nachmansohn  came  to  the  United  States,  in  the  summer 
of  1939,  under  the  sponsorship  of  the  Dazian  Foundation,  and  since  this 
is  something  of  a  family  party,  I  will,  perhaps,  be  forgiven  for  telling 
you  a  family  secret.  The  Dazian  Foundation  had  wished  to  sponsor 
a  physiologist  from  Europe.  David  Nachmansohn  was  chosen,  and  I 
can  only  say  that  American  Physiology  has  been  vastly  stimulated  by 
his  presence  in  this  country.  He  and  his  wife  have  made  a  solid  place 
for  themselves  here;  and,  in  the  language  of  George  Eliot,  David, 
"through  his  mild  persistence,  has  urged  Man's  thoughts  to  vaster  is- 
sues." 

We  feel  particularly  fortunate  in  being  able  to  welcome  so  many  dis- 
tinguished colleagues  from  abroad,  this  having  been  made  possible  by 
the  vision  of  the  Rockefeller  Foundation  and  of  the  Commission  for 
Relief  in  Belgium.  Our  colleagues  from  France  bring  us  heartening 
news  of  the  revival  of  their  laboratories  and  of  their  faith  in  the  uni- 
versal fellowship  of  scientific  men.  We  are  also  happy  to  see  Pro- 
fessor Augusto  Pi-Sufier  of  Barcelona  and  Caracas,  and  his  son,  Dr. 
Jaime  Pi-Sufier.  Also,  Arturo  Rosenblueth  from  Mexico.  In  Doctors 
Hober  and  Michealis,  we  have  distinguished  representatives  of  the 

(601) 


602  ANNALS  NEW  YORK  ACADEMY  OF  SCIENCES 

highest  traditions  of  German  science,  men  who  could  never  surrender 
their  faith  in  academic  freedom. 

Frederic  Bremer  exemphfies  all  that  we  most  admire  in  his  country- 
men: loyalty,  self-reliance,  humor,  industry,  and,  with  it  all,  a  burning 
zeal  for  research  that  sustained  him  in  his  vigorous  way  of  life  during 
the  lean  years  through  which  he  has  just  passed.  AVhen  conditions 
in  his  university  laboratory  made  work  impossible  in  1943,  he  retired 
to  the  cellar  of  his  house  for  nearly  two  years,  and  there  continued  his 
experimentation  and  his  writing.  Virtue  cannot  be  enhanced  by  calling 
it  to  public  notice  and  I  do  so  now,  not  to  add  luster  to  Frederic 
Bremer's  position  in  the  world  of  science,  but  rather  that  others  may 
take  inspiration  from  his  faith  and  his  example.  He  states  modestly 
that  he  has  merely  followed  in  the  pathways  of  those  he  seeks  to 
emulate.  Many  of  us  here  share  with  him  a  common  devotion  to  two 
of  his  masters:  one  was  Harvey  Gushing;  the  other,  Sir  Gharles  Sher- 
rington. Sir  Gharles,  whom  Bremer  has  recently  seen,  is  now  in  his 
ninth  decade.  Bremer  found  him  hard  at  work,  bringing  out  a  biog- 
raphy of  Jean  Fernel,  the  sixteenth  century  physician  and  humanist; 
while  he  is  somewhat  crippled  by  arthritis,  his  mind  remains  ever 
vigorous. 

Another  pupil  of  Sherrington  is  John  Eccles,  whose  industry,  like 
that  of  Bremer's,  is  phenomenal.  In  a  very  short  space  of  time,  he  has 
had  eight  children,  and,  not  content  with  bringing  up  a  large  family  at 
home,  he  also  created  a  laboratory  family  of  loyal  associates:  Hebbel 
Hoff  and  David  Lloyd  who  were  his  pupils  at  Oxford,  and  Stephen 
Kuffler  (whom  you  have  all  enjoyed  hearing  at  the  Gonference),  Ber- 
nard Katz,  and  many  younger  men  whose  names  we  are  beginning  to 
see  in  the  literature.  Ghandler  Brooks  of  Baltimore  permits  me  to  tell 
you  that  he,  too,  is  going  presently  to  New  Zealand,  to  experience  for 
a  year  the  stimulating  atmosphere  of  Eccles'  laboratory.  Gharacter- 
istic  of  the  Eccles  family,  Mrs.  Eccles  has  extended  a  cordial  invitation 
to  Dr.  and  Mrs.  Brooks  to  live  with  them  while  they  are  in  Dunedin, 
should  they  have  difficulty  in  finding  accommodations.  I  could  tell 
you  more  about  Jack  Eccles,  but  since  he  is  a  good  friend  of  mine  I 
shall  spare  him,  the  more  so  since  he  knows  much  too  much  about  me  to 
make  it  safe  to  indulge  in  blackmail. 

My  pleasant  duty  in  closing  is  to  propose  a  toast.  Since  we  did  not 
wish  to  obligate  anyone  to  speak,  it  seemed  inappropriate  to  single  out 
our  guests,  for  they  have  been  one  with  the  Gonference.  But  it  has 
seemed  highly  appropriate  to  ask  you  to  drink  a  standing  toast  to  the 
man  who  has  probably  influenced  our  thinking  more  profoundly  than 
anyone  now  living — Sir  Gharles  Sherrington.