Skip to main content

Full text of "Physiological mammalogy"

See other formats


PHYSIOLOGICAL   MAMMALOGY 

VOLUME  I 

Mammalian  Populations 


Physiological  Mammalogy 

VOLUME  I 
Mammalian  Populations 

CONTRIBUTIONS  BY 

JOHN  B.  CALHOUN        J.  J.  CHRISTIAN 


VOLUME  II 
Mammalian  Reactions  to  Stressful  Environments 


PHYSIOLOGICAL 
MAMMALOGY 


EDITED    BY 

WILLIAM  V.  MAYER 

Department  of  Biology,  Wayne  State  University,  Detroit,  Michigan 

and 
RICHARD  G.  VAN  GELDER 

Department  of  Mammalogy,  The  American  Museum  of  Natural  History 
New  York,  New  York 


VOLUME  I 
Mammalian  Populations 


1963 


ACADEMIC  PRESS— New  York  and  London 


COPYKIGHT    ©    1963,    BY    ACADEMIC    PrESS    InC. 


ALL    RIGHTS    RESERVED 

\0  PART  OF  THIS  BOOK  MAY  BE  REPRODUCED  IN  ANY  FORM, 
BY  PHOTOSTAT,  MICROFILM,  OR  ANY  OTHER  MEANS,  WITHOUT 
WRITTEN    PERMISSION    FROM    THE    PUBLISHERS. 


ACADEiMIC  PRESS  INC. 

1 1 1  Fifth  Avoniie,  New  York  o.  New  York 


United  Kingdom  Edition  published  by 
ACADEMIC  PRESS  INC.  (LONDON)  LTD. 
Berkeley'  Square  House,  London  W.l 


Library  of  Congress  Card  Number:  63-23422 


PRINTED    IN    THE    UNITED    ST.\TES    OF    AMERICA 


Contributors  to  Volume  I 


JOHN  B.  CALHOUN 

Laboratory  of  Psychology 

National  Institute  of  Mental  Health 

Bethesda,  Maryland 

J.  J.  CHRISTIAN 

Division  of  Endocrinology  and  Reproduction 

Research  Laboratories,  Albert  Einstein  Medical  Center 

Philadelphia,  Pennsylvania 


PREFACE 

The  field  of  mammalogy  has,  until  very  recently,  been  largely  concerned 
with  morphology  and  systematics;  investigators  have  done  little  to  bring 
together  experimental  work  on  the  widely  divergent  mammalian  types  of 
which  we  have  knowledge.  The  treatise  "Physiological  Mammalogy"  was, 
therefore,  conceived  as  a  device  to  bring  together  the  existing  knowledge 
of  an  experimental  nature  on  those  animals  usually  regarded  as  "wild." 
Approximately  fifteen  themes,  on  which  a  considerable  body  of  evidence 
has  been  accumulated  over  the  years,  provide  the  central  organizing  core 
around  which  various  authors  have  been  asked  to  make  their  contribu- 
tions. The  amount  of  material  available  on  the  non-classic  laboratory 
animals  has  been  difficult  to  synthesize ;  but  the  experts  who  were  asked  to 
contribute  to  the  planned  original  single  volume  have  proven  so  knowl- 
edgeable about  the  areas  of  their  specialities  and  have  made  such  a  truly 
impressive  survey  of  the  experimental  literature  on  their  topics  that  it 
has  become  necessary  to  expand  the  work  from  the  original  single  volume 
of  relatively  short  essays  to  a  minimum  of  three  and  perhaps  even  more 
additional  volumes. 

The  plan  of  this  work  is  such  that  anyone  working  with  mammals  will 
find  it  an  indispensable  reference.  It  is  particularly  valuable  to  experi- 
mentalists working  with  mammals  in  the  areas  of  physiology,  mammalogy, 
and  ecolog3^  Within  these  volumes  will  be  found  comprehensive  essays  on 
specific  topics  in  physiological  mammalogy,  as  well  as  a  cogent  analysis  of 
the  experimental  field  developing  both  what  is  known  and  what  needs  yet 
to  be  done.  The  research  worker  will  find,  in  perusing  these  pages,  chal- 
lenging obser\'ations  to  which  he  might  well  address  future  researches. 
The  student  will  find  reference  material  and  previous  observations  which 
will  make  these  volumes  useful  as  a  baseline  from  which  additional  studies 
can  be  begun.  ]\Iost  of  the  authors  have,  in  addition  to  providing  a  com- 
prehensive review  article,  intercalated  their  own  observations  and  com- 
mentary to  the  point  where  the  articles  themselves  constitute  original 
contributions  to  the  field.  This  treatment  provides  a  comprehensive  analy- 
sis of  the  over-all  specified  topic. 

These  volumes  will  provide  the  investigator  with  information  that 
will  enable  him  to  choose  experimental  animals  previously  little  used  in  the 
laboratory  because  their  specific  physiological  properties  and  behavior 
were  formerly  not  well  known.  As  a  compendium  on  physiology  of  nor- 
mally non-laboratory  mammals,  this  treatise  will  be  of  value  to  anyone 
who  works  with  mammals  and  to  any  experimental  animal  biologist. 


viii  Preface 

The  first  volume  consists  of  two  comprehensive  articles  dealing  with  the 
physiology  of  populations.  Calhoun's  "The  Social  Use  of  Space"  presents 
many  interesting  new  ideas  on  the  behavior  of  animals  in  populations  and 
the  effects  of  grouping  of  individuals  upon  the  physiology  of  the  organism. 
Where  many  workers  have  thought  of  the  experimental  animal  only  as  an 
individual  apparently  divorced  from  his  environment  and  other  members 
of  the  species,  Calhoun  points  out  the  fallacy  of  this  view  in  providing 
experimental  data  that  demonstrate  the  effects  of  numbers  of  individuals 
on  the  behavior  of  each  individual.  Christian's  article  on  population  growth 
treats  the  problem  largely  from  an  ecological  viewpoint  in  dealing  with 
limiting  factors  of  natural  populations  and  population  interrelationships. 

Volume  II  will  consist  of  three  contributions  dealing  with  natural 
populations  and  their  adaptations  to  stressful  environments.  Dr.  Charles 
Kayser  treats  the  mammalian  phenomenon  of  hibernation  as  a  mechanism 
for  avoiding  periods  of  unfavorable  environment,  and  Dr.  Robert  Chew 
deals  with  water  balance  in  desert  rodents.  Inasmuch  as  reproduction  is 
considerably  affected  by  the  environment,  it  too  can  be  considered  a  process 
modified  by  environmental  stresses,  and  is  discussed  in  the  second  volume. 
Subsequent  volumes  will  include  articles  on  such  topics  as  temperature  and 
metabolism,  physiological  genetics,  photoperiod,  and  orientation  by  echo- 
location.  The  Editors  will  conclude  the  series  with  a  summary  article  on 
the  phylogeny  of  physiology. 

The  Editors  feel  particularly  fortunate  in  that  they  have  received  fine 
cooperation  from  outstanding  authorities  in  the  specific  subject  matter 
topics  covered.  It  is  the  caliber  of  the  individual  author  on  which  these 
volumes  base  their  contribution  to  science;  and  while  the  Editors  assume 
any  responsibility  for  defects  of  organization  or  inadvertent  errors,  the 
credit  for  the  success  of  the  volumes,  as  a  whole,  rests  on  the  indi^'idual 
contributors. 

October,  1963  William  V.  Mayer 

Richard  G.  Van  Gelder 


INTRODUCTION 

The  development  of  the  biological  sciences  has  been  largely  through 
the  observational  method.  In  the  nineteenth  century  descriptive  biologists 
came  to  occupy  a  preeminent  and  leading  position  in  the  field  of  biology. 
With  the  advent  of  the  twentieth  century,  however,  biology  began  to 
emphasize  the  experimental;  and  today,  biology  is  an  experimental  science 
in  practically  all  of  its  branches.  However,  experimentation  in  animal 
biology  has  very  largely  concentrated  on  a  relatively  few  species  which 
have  come  to  be  classic.  These  animals  were  selected  because  of  their 
tractability  and  availability,  and  because  of  the  fund  of  existing  knowledge 
about  them.  Therefore,  animal  experimental  biology  has  been  largely 
oriented  around  the  dog,  cat,  rabbit,  mouse,  guinea  pig,  and  white  rat. 
Despite  the  fact  that  again  and  again  it  has  become  obvious  that  there 
are  wide  varieties  of  metabolic  patterns  and  inter-specific  difl^erences  in 
physiology  as  well  as  morphology,  the  bulk  of  experimentation  continues 
to  be  done  with  relatively  few  classic  types.  Despite  this  concentration  of 
effort,  over  the  years  numerous  researchers  have  investigated  the  use  of 
different  animals  for  experimental  purposes,  including  mammals  ranging 
from  aardvarks  to  zebras. 

The  writings  of  these  investigators,  however,  are  scattered  widely  both 
in  literature  and  in  time,  making  it  diflScult  for  an  experimental  biologist 
to  draw  on  the  fund  of  already  available  knowledge  about  animals  other 
than  those  most  frequently  used.  The  basic  thesis  guiding  the  preparation 
of  "Physiological  Mammalogy"  has  been  to  make  available  to  the  experi- 
mental biologist  the  wealth  of  data  in  the  work  of  widely  geographically 
scattered  research  scientists  of  diverse  experimental  interests. 


:S\ca7 


TABLE  OF  CONTENTS 


LIST    OF    CONTRIBUTORS V 

PREFACE Vii 

INTRODUCTION ix 

CHAPTER   1 

The  Social  Use  of  Space 
John  B.  Calhoun 

I.     Introduction 2 

II.     The  Bivariate  Normal  Type  of  Home  Range 4 

III.  Behavioral  Origins  of  the  Bivariate  Normal  Type  of  Home  Range 8 

IV.  Use  of  a  Two-Dimensional  Field 19 

V.     Summary  of  the  Concept  of  Home  Range 25 

VI.     Continuous  Removal  Trapping  of  Small  Mammals 26 

VII.     Toward  a  General  Theorj^  of  Interspecific  and  Intraspecific  Use 

of  Space 34 

VIII.     Interpretations  of  Observed  Data  Derived  from  Removal  Trapping 

of  Small  Mammals 52 

IX.     A  Theoretical  Conceptualization  of  the  Evolution  of  a  Social 

Hierarchy  among  Species  in  the  Utilization  of  Space 70 

X.     Psychological  Dominance  as  the  Primary  Component  of  the  Niche 77 

XI.     An  Induced  Invasion 80 

XII .     Derivation  of  Compact  Colonies  from  Constellations 86 

XIII.  A  Formulation  of  Group  Dynamics 101 

XIV.  Consequences  and  Examples  of  Social  Interaction  Systems 148 

XV.     Conclusion 184 

References 185 

xi 


xii  Table  of  Contents 

CHAPTER   2 

Endocrine  Adaptive  Mechanisms  and  the 
Physiologic  Regulation  of  Population  Growth 

J.  J.  Christian 

General  Introduction 189 

PART    1.     THE    ENDOCRINE    ADAPTIVE    MECHANISMS 

I .     Introduction 191 

II.     The  Endocrine  Glands  of  Adaptation 192 

PART   2.    PHYSIOLOGIC    ADAPTION    AND    MAMMALIAN   POPULATIONS 

I.     Introduction 261 

II.     Endocrine  Responses  to  Social  Pressures  and  to  Population  Density 263 

III.     Conclusion 325 

References 328 

AUTHOR   INDEX 355 

SUBJECT     INDEX 363 


The  Social  Use  of  Space 


JOHN  B.  CALHOUN 

Laboratory  of  Psydwlogy,  National  Institute  of  Mental  Health,  Bethesda,  Maryland 


TABLE  OF  CONTENTS 

I.  Introduction 2 

II.  The  Bivariate  Normal  Type  of  Home  Range 4 

III.  Behavioral  Origins  of  the  Bivariate  Normal  Type  of  Home  Range.  _  8 
A.  Activity  in  a  One-Dimensional  Habitat 8 

IV.  Use  of  a  Two-Dimensional  Field 19 

A.  Theoretical  Origin 19 

B.  Travel-Path  Home  Range 24 

V.  Summary  of  the  Concept  of  Home  Range 25 

VI.  Continuous  Removal  Trapping  of  Small  Mammals 26 

A.  Rich  Lake  Island,  New  York,  1952,  Sixty-Day  Removal  Study — 

Data  Contributed  by  William  L.  Webb 27 

B.  Chadwick  Woods,  Montgomery  County,  Maryland,  Removal 

Study,  1958-1959— Data  Contributed  by  Kyle  R.  Berbehenn  29 

C.  Comparative  Catches,  Huntington  Wildlife  Forest,  1952-1953 — 

Data  Contributed  by  Earl  F.  Patric  and  WilUam  L.  Webb_  .  .  29 

D.  Comparative  Catches,  Huntington  Wildlife  Forest,  1951 — Data 

Contributed  by  William  L.  Webb 30 

E.  Comparative  Catches  in  Maine  (1950)  and  Maryland  (1953) ...  31 

F.  Comparative  Catches  of  Peromyscus  and  Clethrionomys 32 

VII.  Toward  a  General  Theory  of  Interspecific  and  Intraspecific  Use  of 

Space 34 

A.  A  Two-Species  System 36 

B.  The  Nature  of  the  Inhibitory  Influence 38 

C.  The  Learning  of  Signals 39 

D.  The  Distance  between  Neighbors  of  the  Same  Species 42 

E.  Methods  of  Calculating  Data  Relative  to  the  Distance  between 

Neighbors 44 

F.  Further  Comment  on  the  Impact  of  All  Individuals  on  the 

Environment 44 

G.  Contacting  Neighbors 45 

H.  Sign  Field  of  All  Neighbors 47 

I.    Signal  Field  of  Neighbors 47 

J.    Hum  Field 50 

K.  General  Conclusion  Concerning  the  Distance  between  Neighbors  50 

L.  The  Number  of  Neighbors  Perceived 51 

1 


John  B.  Calhoun 

VIII.  Interpretations  of  Observed  Data  Derived  from  Removal  Trapping 

of  Small  Mammals 52 

A.  The  Relationship  between  Two  Dominant  Species 52 

B.  Removal  Captures  of  Socially  Dominant  Species 55 

C.  Constellation  Formation — An  I ntraspecific  Phenomenon 57 

D.  Expected  Variability  in  the  Number  of  Individuals  Forming 

Constellations 62 

E .  Social  Rank  and  Intraspecific  Associations 64 

F.  The  Instability  of  Social  Relations 67 

IX.  A   Theoretical    Conceptualization   of   the   Evolution   of   a   Social 

Hierarchy  among  Species  in  the  Utilization  of  Space 70 

X.  Psycholo.2;ical  Dominance  as  the  Primary  Component  of  the  Niche. .  77 

XI.  An  Induced  Invasion 80 

XII.  Derivation  of  Compact  Colonies  from  Constellations 86 

A.  Compact  Colony  Formation  in  the  Norway  Rat 87 

B.  Howler  Monkeys,  a  Compact  Colony  Living  Species 90 

C.  Behavioral  Sink  D;>velopment  by  the  Norway  Rat 92 

D.  Yarding  by  Deer  in  Northern  Wisconsin 95 

E.  Concerning  Basic  Numl;ers,  A^d,  for  Man 97 

XIII.  A  Formulation  of  Group  Dyi  amies 101 

A.  The  Model  of  Social  Intera.'tion 101 

B.  Basic  Processes  Involved  in  Social  Interaction 116 

XIV.  Consequences  and  Examples  of  Social  Interaction  Systems 148 

A.  Velocity  Reduction  in  a  Hierarchy  of  Mice 148 

B.  The  Choosing  of  a  Partner 154 

C.  The  Response-Evoking  Capacity  Circumplex 155 

D.  Conformity,  Withdrawal,  and  Creativity 162 

E.  Velocity  and  Home  Range 164 

F.  Velocity  in  High-Density  Rat  Societies 168 

G.  Exploratory  Behavior 175 

XV.  Conclusion 184 

References 185 


I.  Introduction 

During  recent  years  many  investigators  have  considered  the  physiological 
consequences  to  the  individual  of  altering  the  size  of  the  group  of  which  it 
is  a  member.  Inherent  in  such  studies  is  the  assumption  that  for  a  particular 
species  there  is  some  optimum  group  size,  above  or  below  which  the  altered 
frecjuency  or  type  of  interactions  are  either  stressful  or  fail  to  elicit  optimum 
physiological  states.  My  purpose  wall  be  to  develop  formulations  con- 
cerning the  social  use  of  space  to  determine  whether  there  might  be  certain 
optimum  group  sizes. 

I  shall  consider  only  indirectly  the  physiology  of  individuals.  Instead, 
emphasis  will  be  upon  relationships  which  determine  the  "physiology"  of 
the  community.  The  basic  particle  is  the  individual  mammal.  In  any  total 


1.  The  Social  Use  of  Space  3 

assembly  of  such  particles  inhabiting  a  particular  environment,  taxonomic 
categories,  such  as  species  and  genera,  represent  general  classes  of  particles. 
Although  several  classes  may  share  certain  characteristics,  each  possesses 
characteristics  peculiar  to  itself,  which  on  the  average  differentiate  it  from 
all  other  classes.  Furthermore,  the  properties  of  any  particular  particle 
may  change  through  time  as  a  function  of  maturation  and  experience. 
Such  changes  are  reflected  in  the  individual's  internal  milieu.  Only  at  this 
level  are  we  concerned  with  physiology  in  its  classical  sense. 

A  social  system  consists  of  particles  moving  through  space  and  time.  In 
the  course  of  evolution  and  maturation,  such  particles  may  develop  en- 
hanced capacities  for  affecting  others  and,  in  turn,  for  being  more  affected 
by  them.  This  inquiry  seeks  that  essential  nature  of  these  particles  which 
influences  the  course  of  social  evolution.  It  concludes  that  each  category  of 
particles  which  we  designate  as  a  species  must  develop  a  basic  group  size, 
designated  as  its  basic  number,  Nb.  Then,  assuming  the  validity  of  the 
concept  of  an  Nb,  the  inquiry  is  extended  to  encompass  the  following 
questions : 

1.  How  do  changes  in  TV  from  Nb  alter  the  social  system? 

2.  Given  a  species,  whose  groups  are  of  the  optimum  Nb  type,  how  do 
changes  in  the  environment  or  in  the  nature  of  the  particles  affect 
the  social  system? 

3.  What  are  the  basic  organizational  and  interactional  properties  of  the 
group? 

I  found  very  few  prior  studies  in  the  literature  suitable  for  guiding  me 
in  developing  formulations  adequate  to  satisfy  these  objectives.  Therefore, 
this  discourse  will  be  neither  a  review  of  the  literature  nor  even  an  adequate 
presentation  of  empirical  data.  Instead,  the  major  theme  concerns  develop- 
ment of  a  logically  sound,  theoretical  framework  of  processes  underlying 
social  phenomena. 

In  the  course  of  this  development  some  empirical  data  will  be  presented. 
These  data  are  not  meant  to  prove  the  correctness  of  the  formulations. 
They  are  presented  simply  as  background  for  the  origin  of  ideas  or  to  show 
that  there  at  least  exist  some  data  which  are  harmonious  with  the  developed 
concepts.  Much  of  such  data  is  presented  here  for  the  first  time.  I  am  in- 
debted to  many  persons  for  their  permission  to  cite  such  original  data. 

I  have  attempted  to  place  many  of  the  concepts  in  sufficiently  precise 
mathematical  form  to  permit  a  better  understanding  of  the  dynamics  in- 
volved. My  inadequacy  in  the  field  of  mathematics  has  been  buffered  by 
the  advice  of  several  competent  mathematicians :  James  U.  Casby,  Murray 
Eden,   Samuel  W.   Greenhouse,   Seymour  Geisser,   Clifford  Patlak,   and 


4  John  B.  Calhoun 

John  Gilbert.  However,  I  assume  full  responsibility  for  any  errors,  in- 
adequate presentation,  or  overextension  from  their  initial  guidance. 

I  have  found  this  effort  a  rewarding  one  for  the  development  of  insight 
into  complex  social  systems,  and  I  can  only  hope  that  in  some  small  meas- 
ure it  may  serve  as  a  bridge  for  others  in  their  design  of  experiments  or  in 
their  further  theorizing. 


II.  The  Bivariate  Normal  Type  of  Home  Range 

Home  range  denotes  the  area  covered  by  an  individual  in  its  day-to- 
day activities.  Field  studies  of  many  species  of  mammals  have  revealed 
that  each  individual  customarily  stays  within  a  restricted  area  for  long 
periods.  The  individual  utilizes  the  center  of  such  an  area  most  inten- 
sively. With  increasing  radial  distance  from  this  home  range  center  (HRC) 
the  relative  frequency  of  visitation  per  unit  of  area  decreases.  Calhoun 
and  Casby  (1958)  found  that  the  bivariate  normal  distribution  function 
adequately  describes  home  range.  The  following  is  a  summary  of  their 
analyses. 

In  home  range  studies,  "density  function"  is  a  mathematical  expression 
representing  the  probability  of  an  animal  being  present  in  some  arbitrarily 
small  area.  Three  assumptions  are  made : 

The  home  range  is  fixed.  In  other  words,  the  statistics  of  the  home  range 
are  stationary  or  time  independent. 

There  is  a  true  center  of  activity  although  the  apparent  center,  the  mean 
coordinate  point  of  capture,  of  activity  may  deviate  from  it. 

The  probability  of  an  animal  being  in  a  unit  of  area  decreases  with  in- 
creasing distances  from  the  true  center  of  activity.  This  and  the  second 
assumption  suggest  a  bivariate  normal  distribution  of  the  density  function : 

f{x,  7j)  dxdy  =  — — -  exp  [-  (a:^  +  if)/2a^']  dxdy  (1) 

where  o-  is  the  standard  deviation  of  the  distances  in  the  x  and  y  direction 
and  is  assumed  to  be  equal  for  both,  and  x  and  y  are  measured  from  their 
respective  means.  This  density  function  may  be  used  to  represent  the 
percentage  of  time  spent  in  the  area  dxdy  located  at  the  Cartesian  coordi- 
nates X,  y,  or  in  polar  coordinates: 

/(/•,  6)  rdedr  =  — —  exp  (-r'^/'Ia^)  rdddr  (2) 

zcrV 

Here,  the  area  rdddr  is  determined  by  r. 

The  density  function  in  terms  of  the  Cartesian  coordinates  is  more 


1.   The  Social  Use  of  Space 


meaningful  from  an  ecological  standpoint  because  it  states  in  comparative 
terms  the  amount  of  time  spent  by  an  animal  in  a  small  standard  area  at 
any  position  in  the  home  range.  However,  for  the  initial  mathematical 
manipulation,  it  was  found  more  convenient  to  express  the  density  func- 
tion in  terms  of  polar  coordinates.  Then  the  probability  of  finding  the 
animal  between  the  radii  r  and  r  -\-  dr  about  the  true  center  of  the  home 
range  is: 


2 

f{r)dr  =  — -  exp  (  —  r~/2a'^)  rdr 


If  Eq.  (3)  is  integrated  over  the  range  0  to  o-  we  have 

r  2r 

/    T-  exp  (-rV2(r2)  rfr  =  1  -  g-'/^  =  0.3940 


'o    -^0- 


(3) 


(4) 


In  the  above  eciuations  a,  the  standard  deviation  of  the  normal  distribu- 
tion function,  is  the  value  of  a  radius  within  w^hich  the  probability  of  the 
animal  being  present  is  39.4%,  if  its  movements  can  be  described  by  a 
bivariate  normal  density  function. 

Similarly,  integrating  Eq.  (3)  over  the  range  0  to  2o-  gives 

1  -  e-4/2  ^  0.8645  (5) 

Similarly,  integrating  Eq.  (3)  over  the  range  0  to  3o-  gives 


1  _  p-9/2  =  0. 


f6) 


The  above  sigma  thus  delineates  a  single  distance  term  by  which  home 
range  may  be  described.  The  term  "home  range  sigma"  will  be  so  utilized 
in  following  sections. 

Although  this  sigma  may  be  calculated  from  a  series  of  coordinate  points 
of  capture  by  equations  presented  in  the  original  paper,  use  of  recapture 
radii  provide  a  more  direct  means,  adequate  for  most  purposes.  Calculate 
the  mean  coordinate  point  of  capture,  the  approximate  home  range  center. 
Then  on  a  large  scale  grid  map  of  the  study  area  measure  recapture  radii, 
r,  from  this  mean  coordinate  point  of  capture.  Unbiased  estimates  of  sigma, 
s  and  Si  may  be  calculated  by  the  following  equations: 


s  = 


&i  = 


_2(A^  -  n). 

Ki 
3=1 

_2(/:.-  1) 

(7) 


(8) 


6  John  B.  Calhoun 

where : 

s  =  unbiased  estimate  of  the  home  range  sigma  for  all  the  animals 

in  a  sample 
St  =  unbiased  estimate  of  the  home  range  sigma  for  any  particular 
animal 
Ki  =  number  of  captures  of  ith  animal 
n  =  number  of  animals 

n 

N  =  total  captures  =  ^  Ki 

ij  =  jih  observation  of  the  iih.  animal 

A  detailed  analysis  of  the  home  range  for  25  male  harvest  mice 
(Reithrodontomys)  on  which  there  were  10  to  24  captures  each  indicated 
that  there  was  a  significant  variation  of  sigma  among  animals.  In  other 
words,  some  animals  had  significantly  larger  home  ranges  than  others. 
Therefore,  in  order  to  compare  the  observed  recapture  radii  with  the 
theoretical  (Table  II  in  Calhoun  and  Casby,  1958),  each  recapture  radius 
was  normalized  into  a  standard  measure  denoted  by  Z  in  which  the  home 
range  sigma  for  each  animal  was  assigned  a  value  of  1.0  and  all  recapture 
radii  expressed  as  a  proportion  of  this. 

As  may  be  seen  from  Fig.  1,  the  theoretical  closely  approximated  the 
observed.  Although  this  detailed  analysis  has  been  applied  only  to  this  one 
species,  it  shall  be  assumed  for  the  purpose  of  developing  further  formula- 
tion that  the  bivariate  normal  distribution  function  adequately  describes 
fixed  home  ranges  of  other  species. 

Comparison  of  observed  and  theoretical  distribution  of  home  range 
radii  required  viewing  home  range  as  a  probability  of  capture  which  changes 
with  radial  distance  from  the  home  range  center.  Bands  of  equal  width 
increase  in  area  with  radial  distance  from  the  home  range  center,  while 
probability  of  capture  per  unit  area  decreases  with  increase  in  radial  dis- 
tance. Interaction  of  these  two  factors  results  in  more  captures  at  about 
one  sigma  from  the  home  range  center  than  at  any  other  distance  (Fig.  1). 

However,  the  ecologically  important  aspect  of  the  bivariate  normal  dis- 
tribution as  an  expression  of  home  range  is  the  relative  probability  that  an 
animal  will  be  in  a  unit  of  area  with  respect  to  the  radial  distance  of  that 
unit  area  from  the  home  range  center  (Fig.  2).  For  any  given  sigma  char- 
acterizing a  particular  species,  its  density  function  in  terms  of  area  curve 
may  be  obtained  by  multiplying  the  relative  sigma  value  on  the  abscissa 
by  the  observed  sigma  and  dividing  the  density  function  values  on  the 
ordinate  by  the  square  of  the  observed  sigma. 


1.   The  Social  Use  of  Space 

70 1 r 


Rodius   from  center  of    home    range 

Fig.  1.  Observed  (histogram)  and  theoretical  distribution  of  348  recapture  radii 
(Z)  of  25  male  harvest  mice  from  the  center  of  their  home  range.  Z  here  represents  a 
normalized  measure  of  the  bivariate  normal  home  range  sigma. 


0  .6  1.2  1.8  2.4  3.0 

Radius    from   center  of  home  ronge    in  units   of  cr 


3.6 


Fig.  2.   Cross  section  of  the  density  function  of  home  range  in  terms  of  area.  Rotation 
of  this  curve  about  its  axis  reveals  the  mountain-shaped  topography  of  home  range. 


8  John  B.  Calhoun 

III.  Behavioral  Origins  of  the  Bivariate  Normal  Type  of  Home  Range 

The  fact  that  a  particular  equation  happens  to  describe  home  range 
enables  derivation  of  several  principles  regarding  the  use  of  space  by  an 
entire  community.  Discussion  of  these  principles  follows  in  Section  VII. 
However,  as  a  background  to  this  discussion  it  will  be  advantageous  to 
seek  an  understanding  of  the  biological  basis  for  the  bivariate  normal  type 
of  home  range. 

A.  Activity  in  a  One-Dimensional  Habitat 

Admittedly,  animals  rarely  live  in  essentially  one-dimensional  environ- 
ments. However,  I  suspected  that  if  animals  were  placed  in  such  environ- 
ments certain  regularities  of  behavior  might  be  revealed  which  would  pro- 
vide insight  into  their  use  of  two-dimensional  environments.  To  this  end, 
four  14-foot  long  alleys  were  constructed.  Each  had  a  channel  8  inches 
wide.  Each  8  X  12-inch  segment  of  the  floor  was  so  suspended  that  when 
a  domesticated  Norway  rat,  used  as  a  subject,  stood  on  such  a  segment  a 
microswitch  closed.  This  closure  initiated  a  signal  such  that  the  exact 
position  at  every  point  in  time  was  recorded  on  a  recording  oscillograph.  A 
partition  between  the  first  and  second  treadles  formed  a  home  compart- 
ment. A  3  X  3-inch  opening  through  this  partition  provided  access  to  the 
rest  of  the  alley.  Food  and  water  placed  in  this  compartment  further  en- 
hanced the  role  of  this  compartment  as  a  "home."  A  ground  glass  plate, 
through  which  shone  the  light  from  a  100-watt  lamp,  formed  the  opposite 
end  of  the  alley.  This  light  served  to  concentrate  the  activities  of  the  rats 
emanating  from  the  home  compartment.  It  was  as  if  every  foot  of  the  alley 
were  several  feet  long.  Details  of  the  effect  of  varying  light  intensity  at  the 
end  of  the  alley  on  explorations  will  be  presented  elsewhere.  This  ap- 
paratus is  referred  to  as  the  Ferguson  Activity  Alley. 

Suffice  it  to  consider  the  results  from  73  rats,  each  run  for  72  hours  in 
the  alley.  Each  rat  made  from  10  to  30  excursions  out  into  the  alley  each 
night.  Despite  the  presence  of  the  bright  light  at  the  end  of  the  alley,  one- 
fourth  of  the  trips  terminated  at  the  end  of  the  alley.  In  other  words,  the 
end  of  the  alley  formed  a  barrier.  Most  of  the  trips  thus  terminated  at  the 
end  of  the  alley  presumably  would  have  represented  trips  of  greater  length 
had  the  alley  only  been  longer.  Most  of  the  time  a  rat  would  go  out  to  some 
intermediate  distance,  stop  momentarily,  and  then  turn  around  and  go 
directly  back  home.  Occasionally,  a  rat  would  wander  back  and  forth  from 
the  point  of  initial  termination.  All  trips  with  such  vacillations  and  those 
ending  at  the  barrier  were  excluded  from  the  initial  analysis. 


1.   The  Social  Use  of  Space  9 

1.  Trip-Terminations  in  an  Unstructured  One-Dimensional 
Habitat 

The  initial  investigation  focused  upon  examination  of  the  distances  from 
the  home  compartment  at  which  nonvacillating  trips  terminated  in  the 
unstructured  alley.  "Unstructured"  denotes  the  absence  along  the  alley 
of  any  stimuli  likely  to  elicit  responses  and  so  induce  a  rat  to  stop.  Rats 
were  placed  in  the  alley  during  the  middle  of  the  afternoon,  a  time  of 
minimal  activity  within  their  normal  24-hour  rhythm  of  activity.  And  yet 
when  placed  in  the  alley  every  rat  exhibited  a  2-  to  3-hour  period  of  hyper- 
activity. Further  details  of  the  decay  curve  of  this  hyperactivity  are  dis- 
cussed in  Section  III,  A,  3. 

For  73  rats  complete  records   (Table  la)   were  available  for  all  trip- 


Tablk  la 
Number  of  Trips  Terminated  with  Reference  to  Distance  from  Home 


Unstructured  alley 

Structured  alley 

T^'      A 

feet  of 

During  first 

During  next  3 

Strips  of 

Pellets 

termination 

2.5  hours 

6  A.M.-6  P.M. 

periods 

paper 

of  food 

1 

274 

558 

1053 

955 

2 

204 

407 

839 

643 

3 

150 

315 

723 

509 

4 

104 

277 

302 

375 

5 

99 

294 

194 

288 

6 

81 

219 

230 

258 

7 

56 

139 

151 

163 

8 

58 

115 

149 

137 

9 

47 

98 

112 

119 

10 

39 

113 

57 

104 

V 

1112" 

2535" 

3810 

3551 

Barrier  and  vacillat- 

502 

1458 

59* 

127* 

ing  trips 

Total  trips 

1614 

4023 

3869 

3678 

Trips/rat/hour 

8.844 

1..531 

"  Only  nonvacillating  trips  included. 

*  Vacillating  trips  and  trips  at  whose  end  an  object  was  transported  home  are  not 
included  since  the  termination  of  trips  determined  was  solely  by  the  number  of  tran.s- 
I)ortt'd  objects. 


10 


John  B.  Calhoun 


terminations  during  both  the  initial  150  minutes  of  hyperactivity  and  the 
following  three,  6  p.m.  to  6  a.m.,  12-hour  periods  of  normal  heightened 
nocturnal  activity.  The  frequency,  y,  of  terminating  trips  as  a  function  of 
distance,  x,  from  home  is  described  by  the  equation : 

y  =  exp  (o  4-  bx)  (9) 

where  h  is  the  slope.  The  slope  for  trip-terminations  during  the  hyper- 
active period,  6i,  is  —0.2099;  while  62,  the  slope  for  trip-terminations  during 
the  36  hours  of  normal  nocturnal  activity,  is  —0.1924.  The  t  test 


61  —  62 


=  -0.682 


VVar.  (61  -  62) 

has  a  p  value  of  0.051  which  indicates  that  slopes  61  and  62  do  not  differ 
significantly. 

Therefore,  it  is  concluded  that  the  neural  mechanism  producing  termina- 
tion of  trips  is  unaltered  by  the  nearly  sixfold  increase  in  the  incidence  of 
initiating  trips  accompanying  initial  exposure  to  a  strange  environment. 
For  this  reason,  the  mean  slope  of  —0.20115,  i.e.,  (61  +  62) /2,  was  fitted  to 
both  these  sets  of  data  shown  in  Fig.  3  as  trips  per  rat  per  hour  for  compara- 
tive purposes  to  emphasize  the  hyperactivity  of  initial  exposure  to  a  strange 
environment. 


2.0 


1.0 


.02 


"eMOTlONAL  " 
I  si.  2.5  hours 
of  Hyperaclivily 


10 


TERMINATION  OF  TRIP  IN  FEET 


Fig.  3.   Frequency  of  terminating  trips  at  successive  distances  from  the  home  com- 
partment in  the  unstructured  Ferguson  Alley.  See  Table  la. 


1.   The  Social  Use  of  Space  11 

2.  The  Role  of  a  Structured  Environment  on  the  Termination 
OF  Trips 

Natural  habitats  possess  structures  which  elicit  responses.  Items  of  food 
and  nesting  material  represent  structures  normally  causing  animals  such 
as  rats  to  terminate  trips.  When  such  items  are  transported  home  the  trip 
resembles  the  nonvacillating  ones  in  the  one-dimensional  alley  in  the  sense 
that  there  is  a  direct  outward  phase,  terminated  by  the  object  being  picked 
up,  followed  by  a  direct  homeward  trip  transporting  the  item.  In  order  to 
explore  the  effect  of  such  structuring  in  the  one-dimensional  habitat  upon 
termination  of  trips,  one  of  two  procedures  was  followed;  At  each  one- 
foot  interval  from  home  along  the  alley,  there  was  placed  a  pad  of  paper 
strips  or  an  open  hopper  of  food  pellets.  During  any  particular  rat's  stay 
of  3-12  days  in  the  alley,  only  nesting  material  or  only  food  pellets  were 
available.  Periodic  replenishment  of  each  source  ensured  a  continuous 
supply  at  each  distance.  Nevertheless,  the  rats  removed  items  from  each 
distance  (Table  la)  even  though  this  necessitated  passing  by  opportunities 
to  respond  while  on  the  outward  journey.  Each  item  removed  at  a  particular 
distance  from  home  is  considered  to  indicate  a  trip-termination  at  that 
distance.  Examination  of  the  oscillograph  record  confirmed  this  inter- 
pretation. 

The  frequency  of  termination  of  such  trips  as  a  function  of  distance  is 
also  described  by  the  equation,  y  =  exp  (a  -\-  bx) .  The  slope  for  trips  termi- 
nated by  picking  up  paper  strips,  63,  is  —0.3027;  while  64,  the  slope  relating 
to  securing  food  pellets,  is  —0.2481.  The  t  test, 

63  —  hi 

=  -2.128 


VVar.  (63  -  64) 

has  a  p  value  between  0.05  and  0.01  which  indicates  a  statistically  signifi- 
cant difference  between  these  two  slopes.  However,  examination  of  Fig.  4 
reveals  a  marked  dispersion  about  the  best-fit  line  of  the  observed  points 
relating  to  nesting  material.  For  this  reason,  the  interpretation  that  the 
63  and  64  slopes  differ  statistically  is  open  to  question  that  this  difference 
in  slope  implies  biological  significance.  I  therefore  believe  it  wisest  to  as- 
sume that  63  and  64  are  really  identical,  or  nearly  so. 

If  this  is  so,  we  may  compare  the  slopes  of  the  mean  of  61  +  62  with  that 
of  63  -f  64.  Here  the  t  test 

Uh  +  62)  -  1(63  +  6.)  ^  ^^^25 


VVar.  Uh  +  62)  -  K&3  +  64) 
with  a  p  value  less  than  0.001. 


12 


John  B.  Calhoun 


5 

o  1000 


1         1         1         1          1         1         I         1          1 
/I               Paper   strips 

' 

B Food  pellets 

- 

- 

\-v 

~ 

o\ 

" 

- 

- 

— 

— 

- 

• 

- 

~ 

1          1         1         1          1         1         1         1         1 

1 

DISTANCE    FROM  HOME  IN  FEET 
FROM   WHICH   OBJECTS   WERE    TAKEN 

Fig.  4.  Frequency  of  transporting  nesting  material  and  food  into  the  home  com- 
partment from  points  at  successively  greater  distance  from  it.  A,  3810  strips  of  paper; 
B,  3551  pellets  of  food.  See  Table  la. 


It  is  therefore  concluded  that  structuring  the  environment  with  items 
inducing  responses  leads  to  a  reduction  of  the  distance  from  home  at  which 
trips  are  terminated. 


3.  The  Prob.\bility  of  Terminating  Trips 

The  two  prior  sections  merely  demonstrate  an  effect  produced  by  struc- 
turing the  environment.  They  do  not  further  our  understanding  of  the 
underlying  biological  process. 

The  behavior  of  rats  in  the  structured  environment  provides  the  clue. 
During  any  period  of  intensive  transportation,  one  trip  almost  immediately 
followed  the  preceding  one.  And  yet  the  distance  at  which  a  particular 
trip  terminated  bore  no  relationship  to  the  distance  at  which  the  previous 
or  following  one  terminated.  It  was  as  if  the  rat  w^as  blind  to  its  surroundings 
on  the  outward  trip  until  some  neural  switching  mechanism  became  acti- 
vated in  a  random  fashion  with  reference  to  the  time  of  the  trip's  initiation. 
This  switching  on  (or  off?)  placed  the  rat  in  a  perceptive  phase  at  which 
time  it  responded  by  picking  up  the  nearest  relevant  object  and  trans- 
porting it  into  the  home  compartment.  Therefore,  it  will  be  helpful  to  deter- 


1.  The  Social  Use  of  Space  13 

mine  the  probability  of  this  switching,  which  is  synonymous  with  the 
probability  of  terminating  a  trip. 

Let:  tj  =  the  number  of  trips  reaching  any  jth  distance  from  home. 
Nj  =  the  number  of  trips  that  stop  at  the  jth.  distance. 
Pj  =  probability  of  stopping  at  the  jth  distance. 

Then: 

tj~i  -  tj  =  Vi-itj-i  (10) 

Nj  =  p,tj  (11) 

Nj  -  Nj_i  =pjtj  -  pj-it.j^i  (12) 

If  Pj  =  p  (a  constant  independent  of  j),  then: 

Nj  -  Nj^i  =  p(ij  -  ij_i) 

=  -p(tj-i  -  l.j)  (13) 

Nj  -  iVy_i  =    -piptj^i)  (14) 

And  by  analogy  to  Eq.  (11): 

iVy_i  =  pti^i  (15) 

Substituting  Eq.  (15)  into  Eq.  (14): 

Nj  -  Nj^i  =  -pNj^i  (16) 

Therefore 

p  =   (Nj_r  -  Nj)/Nj_r  (17) 

This  p  represents  a  constant  probability  of  terminating  trips  which 
arrive  at  a  point  regardless  of  the  distance  from  home.  Rigorous  proof  that 
this  p  actually  is  a  constant  is  difficult  from  present  data  because  of  the 
barrier  produced  by  the  relatively  short  alley.  However,  the  validity  of  a 
constant  p,  independent  of  distance,  may  be  arrived  at  intuitively  since  an 
equation  of  the  form  y  =  exp  (a  +  hx)  best  represents  the  observed  data. 
In  other  words,  log  ij  plotted  against  x  forms  a  straight  line.  Whenever 
this  is  so,  Eq.  (17)  must  be  true. 

Utilizing  Eq.  (9)  stated  in  the  form : 

loge?/  =  a  -  bx  (18) 

the  expected  number  of  trips  terminating  at  Nj^i  and  Nj,  where  j  =  2, 
were  found  to  be  as  shown  in  Table  lb,  along  with  the  p  values  calculated 
from  Eq.  (17).  Thus,  the  probability  of  0.182  of  terminating  trips  arriving 
at  any  distance  in  the  unstructured  alley  is  increased  to  0.24  by  structuring. 


14 


John  B.  Calhoun 


Table  lb 

Probability  of  Terminating  Trips  with  Respect  to  Environmental  Structure 

AND  Level  of  Activity 


Alley 


Secondary  condition 


Expected  number  of 
trips  terminating  at: 


A^,-i 


A^i 


Mean 


During  first  2.5  hr.  of  hyper-       237.5         194.3         0.182« 

activity 
Unstructured  0 .  182 

During  normal  nocturnal  ac-       510  417  0.182° 

tivity 


Structured 


Paper  transportation 
Food  transportation 


999 


783.3    0.261 


837     653.5    0.220 


0.240 


"  The  mean  b  value  of  —0.20115  used  in  the  calculation  of  p. 


1000 


i     100 


50 


~>s^ 

— 1 

1 

A  ■ 

1         1         1         1         I         1 — — 

/f£  Trips  terminated  Z 

RE  Objects  transported  _ 

- 

\                                          — 

.    1. .. 

1       .       1       .       .       1    ~ 

10 


DISTANCE  FROM  HOME  IN  FEET 


Fig.  5.  Theoretical  curves  depicting  relative  likelihood  of  trips  going  beyond  suc- 
cessive distances  from  home  based  upon  an  initial  1000  departures.  A,  in  an  unstructured 
alley  (see  Fig.  3) ;  B,  in  an  alley  structured  with  objects  available  for  transport  back 
home. 


1.  The  Social  Use  of  Space  15 

The  amount  of  structuring  used  here  increased  the  probability  of  stopping 
by  32%  [i.e.,  (0.240  -  0. 182) /0. 182].  The  extent  to  which  this  difference 
modifies  the  use  of  a  field  may  be  visualized  by  considering  the  following: 
Of  every  1000  trips  arriving  1  foot  from  home  in  the  unstructured  alley,  30 
would  stop  at  the  tenth  foot  and  134  would  continue  farther,  whereas  in 
the  structured  environment  only  20  would  stop  at  the  tenth  foot  and  only 
64  would  continue  farther  (see  Fig.  o). 

In  closing  this  section  it  should  be  emphasized  that  the  probability  of 
stopping  is  basically  a  function  of  time.  In  the  particular  situation,  the  p's 
refer  to  the  time  required  for  the  rats  to  travel  a  distance  of  1  foot. 

A  further  question  may  be  asked:  "How  does  structuring  increase  the 
probability  of  stopping  if  the  animals  are  perceptually  'blind'  to  specific 
stimuli  to  the  extent  of  being  unable  to  exhibit  directed  responses  to  them?" 
One  may  visualize  the  situation  in  general  terms:  There  is  some  assembly 
of  neurons  which  provides  a  signal  terminating  an  ongoing  behavior,  such 
as  outward  locomotion  from  home.  The  magnitude  of  this  signal  neces- 
sary for  behavioral  termination  reciuires  simultaneous  firing  of  some  x 
number  of  neurons.  This  assembly  of  neiu'ons  may  be  called  a  "governor." 
It  must  be  located  anatomically  in  some  subcortical  portion  of  the  brain 
precluding  conscious  awareness  of  its  functioning.  During  any  ongoing 
behavior  all  perceivable  stimuli  unrelated  to  the  ongoing  behavior  initiate 
impulses  which  arrive  at  the  behavior  terminating  governor.  Each  unit  of 
impulse  causes  an  increase  in  the  rate  of  firing  of  the  neuronal  net  forming 
the  governor.  The  greater  the  intensity  of  such  stimuli,  or  the  greater  the 
number  of  stimuli  of  a  given  intensity,  the  more  rapidly  will  the  neurons 
of  the  governor  fire  and  thus  the  shorter  will  be  the  interval  between  emis- 
sion of  effective  signals  by  the  governor.  This  signal  both  terminates  the 
ongoing  behavior  and  produces  awareness  of  stimuli  appropriate  to  eliciting 
those  responses  appropriate  to  initiating  some  other  behavior. 

4.  Initial  Hyperactivity  in  a  Strange  Environment 

For  the  rats  discussed  in  Section  III,  A,  1  the  level  of  activity  in  terms 
of  trips  per  rat  per  hour  (Fig.  3  and  Table  la)  was  approximately  six  times 
as  high  during  the  first  150  minutes  of  exposure  to  a  strange  environment 
in  comparison  with  the  later  periods  following  adjustment.  This  observa- 
tion requires  postulation  of  a  second  governor,  one  which  determines  the 
probability  of  initiating  trips.  Initiating  trips  is  here  considered  to  be 
synonymous  with  initiation  of  periods  of  diffuse  undirected  motor  activity. 
Examination  of  the  expression  of  hyperactivity  during  the  first  2-2| 
hours  of  exposure  to  a  strange  environment  provides  insight  into  the  func- 
tioning of  this  governor  of  trip-initiations. 


16 


John  B.  Calhoun 


For  71  of  the  73  rats  previously  discussed  with  reference  to  the  distaiu^o 
of  trip-terminations,  a  count  was  made  of  the  total  feet  traveled  each  half- 
hour  during  the  first  2|  hours  of  exposure  to  the  alley.  A  similar  analysis 
was  prepared  for  the  26  rats,  considered  further  in  Section  III,  A,  5,  which 
emerged  into  the  alley  more  than  momentarily  and  for  which  the  recording 
system  functioned  properly. 

For  each  set  of  data,  results  are  quite  similar  (Fig.  6).  Activity  con- 
tinuously declines  during  initial  exposure  to  a  strange  environment.  But 
the  important  point  is  that  this  decline  begins  from  an  extremely  elevated 
state  of  hyperactivity.  From  Table  la  it  may  be  calculated  that  the  average 


z  i  100 

K  I 

I-  U.  — 

llJ  tt 

^  X 


*-  < 

Q  2 


10 


1            1           1            1            1 

-                                     4 By  ^/  Flats 

^\.                                B By  26  Rats 

— 

\\. 

— 

- 

\\ 

— 

- 

"^"^^      >v^ 

- 

- 

'x 

- 

N. 

1           1           t           I           1 

12  3  4  5 

CONSECUTIVE  30  MINUTES  IN  ALLEY 


Fig.  6.  Hyperactivity  in  a  strange  environment:  Curve  A  represents  the  mean 
activity  level  of  71  rats  immediately  after  first  exposure  to  the  Ferguson  Alley.  Curve  B 
similarly  gives  the  mean  response  of  26  rats  to  the  NIH  Emotional  Activity  Alley. 


round  trip  approximates  10.44  feet.  During  the  normal  nocturnal  period  of 
activity,  trips  occur  at  the  rate  of  1.58  per  hour.  This  means  that  in  this 
one-dimensional  alley  rats  travel  8  feet  per  half-hour  on  the  average  when 
adjusted  to  their  environment.  As  may  be  seen  from  Fig.  3  the  activity  is 
in(;reased  nearly  twenty  times  normal  during  the  initial  half-hour  in  this 
strange  environment.  Projection  of  the  curves,  shown  in  Fig.  6,  indicates 
that  a  normal  level  of  activity  will  be  reached  by  3—4  hours  of  adjustment. 
Such  decay  curves  of  hyperactivity  suggest  that  the  rats  secrete  some 
humoral  agent  upon  their  initial  exposure  to  strange  stimuli  such  as  repre- 
sented by  the  activity  alley.  This  humoral  substance  increases  the  rate  of 
firing  of  the  neuronal  net  comprising  the  governor  which  determines  onset 
of  diffuse  motor  activity,  such  as  trips  out  into  the  alley.  As  this  substance 
is  degraded,  the  frequency  with  which  this  governor  emits  signals  initiating 
trips  decreases  until  normal  behavior  is  achieved. 


1.  The  Social  Use  of  Space  17 

5.  Avoidance  of  a  Strange  Field 

During  the  pursuit  of  these  studies  on  behavior  in  a  one-dimensional 
habitat,  a  few  rats  failed  to  emerge  into  the  alley  until  after  the  lapse  of 
several  hours.  ]VIost  frequently  such  rats  were  members  of  subgroups 
having  had  less  opportunity  to  adjust  to  novel  stimuli.  This  suggested 
that  the  less  opportunity  an  animal  had  to  make  adjustments  to  strange 
stimuli,  or  the  more  novel  was  the  strange  environment,  the  more  likely 
it  would  be  avoided. 

New  alleys  were  constructed  to  permit  exploration  of  this  hypothesis. 
These  alleys  consisted  of  an  8  X  8-inch  channel  15  feet  long.  At  one  end  a 
home  nest  box  could  be  attached  from  which  the  rat  gained  access  to  the 
alley  by  way  of  a  3  X  3-inch  door.  At  the  opposite  end  of  the  alley,  light 
from  two  60-watt  lamps  shone  through  a  ground  glass  plate.  Unless  other- 
wise modified,  the  floor  was  stationary.  A  photoelectric  cell  at  each  3-foot 
interval  along  each  alley  initiated  a  signal  to  an  Esterline-Angus  event 
recorder  each  time  a  rat  passed.  This  apparatus  is  called  the  "NIH  Emo- 
tional Activity  Alley." 

Each  of  the  7(i  subjects,  Osborne-]Mendel  male  rats,  was  housed  alone 
for  3  months  from  weaning  in  a  smaD  cage  precluding  the  visual  perception 
of  any  object  outside  the  cage.  Cages  were  not  opened.  Food  and  water 
were  delivered  into  the  cage  through  channels  making  it  unnecessary  to 
open  them.  These  procedures  minimized  opportunity  for  adjusting  to  new 
configurations  of  stimuli.  In  the  terminology  of  Section  XIV,  G,  1,  the 
isolation  cage  represents  an  Ei  configuration  of  stimuli.  Under  this  termi- 
nology El  •  • '  En  represent  a  series  of  discrete  configurations  of  stimuli 
which  an  individual  may  encounter  for  the  first  time  in  that  order. 

Twenty  rats  were  transferred  directly  from  the  isolation  cages  into  the 
activity  alley  for  a  2-hour  exposure.  Of  the  remaining  56  rats,  24  were  ex- 
posed to  an  E2  configuration  for  2  hours  for  10  days  prior  to  being  placed 
in  the  alley.  Similarly,  16  others  were  exposed  to  an  E-^  configuration,  while 
the  remaining  16  were  exposed  to  E2  for  2  hours,  then  immediately  exposed 
to  Es  for  2  hours  for  10  days.  Three  days  after  these  exposures,  which  pro- 
vided opportunity  to  adjust  to  the  new  stimuli  of  E2,  E:^,  or  Ei  and  £"3, 
each  rat  was  given  a  2-hour  trial  in  the  activity  alley,  which  represented 
an  Ei  configuration.  Half  of  each  of  these  four  groups  were  placed  in  the 
alley  with  a  stationary  floor,  an  Eu.  configuration;  while  for  the  other  half 
the  alley  contained  sections  of  tilting  floor,  an  E^v,  configuration,  which 
clanged  when  the  rats  passed  over  them. 

Greater  detail  of  these  studies  are  presented  in  Section  XIV,  G,  4  and  in 
Table  lib.  However,  the  results  presented  in  Table  Ila  suflfice  for  the 
present  purposes.  Both  aspects  of  the  hypothesis  were  confirmed.  The 


18 


John  B.  Calhoun 


Table  Ila 

Effect  of  Training  and  Degree  of  Strangeness  of  a  Field  upon  Its  Being 
Entered  during  2  Hours  of  Exposure  by  Each  of  76  Rats" 


B 


'Training"  in  adjusting  to 


Response 

strange  stimuli 

Degree  of  strangene.ss  of  alle}' 

alley 

Yes 

No 

High                        Low 
(tilting  floor)      (stationary  floor) 

Entered 
Avoided 

37 
19 

4 
16 

13                            30 
25                              8 

-A  data:  x^  =  10.8045;  p  less  than  0.001;  B  data;  x''  =  13.7110;  p  less  than  0.005. 

stranger  a  field  or  the  less  opportunity  there  has  been  to  adjust  to  novel 
stimuli,  the  greater  will  be  the  tendency  for  an  animal  to  avoid  that  field. 

6.  Probability  of  Vacillating  at  the  Termination  of  Trips 

In  the  unstructured  Ferguson  Activity  Alley,  rats  usually  went  directly 
out  to  the  point  where  the  trip  terminated,  then  turned  around  and  went 
directly  back  to  the  home  compartment.  Yet  occasionally  when  a  rat 
made  an  excursion  out  into  the  alley,  it  would  wander  back  and  forth 


Table  Hb 

Effect  of  Kind  and  Sequence  of  "Training"  in  Adjustment  to  Strange  Stimuli 
UPON  Avoidance  of  an  Activity  Alley 


Group" 

E,A  Alley 

EiB  Alley 

Total 
trials 

Trials 
entered 

Proportion 
entered 

Total 
trials 

Trials 
entered 

Proportion 
entered 

A 
B 

C 
D 

40 
48 
32 
32 

11 
33 
31 
24 

0.275 
0.688 
0.969 
0.750 

40 

48 
32 
32 

2 

24 

22 

6 

0.050 
0.500 
0.688 
0.188 

Total 

152 

99 

0.651 

152 

54 

0.355 

"  See  Section  XIV,  G,  4  for  details  of  treatment  of  the  four  groups. 


J.  The  Social  Use  of  Space 


19 


about  the  place  where  the  trip  terminated.  Two  hundred  and  fifty-six  of 
the  total  2357  trips  studied  with  reference  to  the  initial  period  of  hyper- 
activity, terminated  in  such  vacillations.  As  graphically  shown  in  Fig.  7 
the  probability  of  vacillating  is  proportional  to  the  length  of  the  trip. 


0  5  10 

LENGTH  OF  TRIP  IN  FEET 
Fig.  7.   Probability  of  trips  including  a  period  of  vacillation,  wandering  back  and 
forth  about  the  point  of  termination.  Curve  based  on  2,357  trips  by  136  rats  during  their 
first  150  minutes  in  the  Fergu.son  Activity-  Alley. 

These  results  are  given  here  because  of  their  bearing  on  the  question  of 
the  proportion  of  time  an  animal  is  capable  of  responding  to  static  compo- 
nents of  its  environment.  As  I  have  stated  above,  I  am  proceeding  with  this 
formulation,  utilizing  the  hypothesis  that  directed  responses  can  be  made 
only  at  the  ends  of  periods  of  diffuse  activity,  which  are  represented  here 
by  the  outward  termination  of  trips.  If  this  responsiveness  at  ends  of  trips 
persists  throughout  the  wandering  about  at  the  end  of  trips,  then  it  follows 
that  the  farther  a  familiar  type  of  object  is  from  home  the  more  likely  it 
will  be  responded  to  in  proportion  to  trips  terminating  there.  This  conclu- 
sion is  subject  to  the  (jualification  that  the  object  is  so  sparsely  distributed 
in  space  as  to  require  more  searching  than  mere  detection  of  the  surround- 
ings at  the  point  of  terminating  a  trip. 

IV.  Use  of  a  Two-Dimensional  Field 

A.  Theoretical  Origin 

We  are  here  considering  animals  whose  movements  emanate  from  a  fixed 
home  base.  For  them  a  two-dimensional  field  differs  from  a  one-dimensional 


20  JoJui  B.  Calhoun 

one  in  that  in  a  two-dimensional  field  the  area  available  at  successive  radial 
distances  from  home  increases  with  radius.  Considering  this  fact,  will  the 
behavior  exhibited  by  rats  in  a  one-dimensional  field  lead  to  an  equation 
for  home  range  closely  resembling  the  bivariate  normal  distribution 
[Eq.    (3)]? 

When  Casby  and  I  originally  found  that  the  bivariate  normal  distribution 
did  conform  with  the  observed  home  range  resulting  from  captures,  we 
were  merely  culminating  a  search  for  a  means  of  describing  the  distribution 
of  captures  about  the  mean  coordinate  point  of  capture.  This  conformity 
revealed  nothing  about  the  biological  mechanisms  involved.  We  shall  now 
inquire  whether  the  phenomena  of  (a)  decreasing  frequency  of  arriving  at 
successively  greater  distances  from  home,  and  (b)  the  probability  of 
wandering  increasing  with  distance  from  home  suffice  to  explain  the  origin 
of  the  bivariate  normal  type  home  range. 

A  critical  issue  concerns  the  origin  of  the  observation  or  "capture."  Two 
types  of  observation  are  possible.  First,  the  observer  may  record  the 
physical  presence  of  the  animal  at  successive  points  independent  of  the 
activities  of  the  individual.  Second,  the  observation  may  arise  as  a  conse- 
quence of  the  animal  responding  to  an  object  placed  by  the  investigator. 
Captures  in  traps  represent  this  type  of  observation. 

An  assumption  is  made  regarding  where  responses,  such  as  entering 
baited  traps,  will  be  made.  This  is  that  such  responses  to  continuously 
present  and  unvarjdng  stimuli  occur  only  during  the  period  of  wandering 
at  the  end  of  trips.  This  assumption  implies  that  the  animal  remains  in  a 
perceptually  blind  state  during  the  outward  and  return  phases  of  a  trip. 
The  circumstantial  evidence  suggesting  this  assumption  will  not  be  con- 
sidered here. 

Let :  Pi  =  probability  of  terminating  a  trip  at  radius  r. 
t  =  time  spent  wandering  at  r  if  it  stops  there. 
C  =  probability  of  capture  at  r,  which  equals  tPi  times  geometry 
factor  of  two-dimensional  space. 

Then: 

Pi(r)  =  Ae-^'-^  (19) 

t{r)  =  Br  (20) 

These  two  equations,  in  which  .4,  B,  and  a  are  constants,  represent  the 
two  basic  assiunptions  regarding  use  of  one-dimensional  space.  Then 
considering  the  geometry  factor: 

C  =  Kre-'i"  '  rdrde  (21) 


1.   The  Social  Use  of  Space  21 

And  normalizing,  it  is  found  that: 

C  =  ^ —  rh'-'-'"  dr  (22) 

Where  r/a  =  R,  the  cumulative  probability  of  capture,  Cum,  as  a  func- 
tion of  the  radius  R  from  home  becomes : 

Cumii  =  1  -  f  —  +  /?  +  1  j  e-«  (23) 

Equation  (23)  above  will  be  called  Curve  II  as  shown  in  Table  III  and 
Fig.  8.  It  may  be  compared  to  a  similar  cumulative  probability  curve  for 
the  bivariate  normal  distribution,  which  will  be  called  Curve  I,  and  which 
has  the  form: 

Cumi  =  1  -  exp  (-rV2)  (24) 

Curve  II,  Eci.  (23),  may  be  compared  to  Cui've  I,  Eq.  (24),  by  con- 
verting R  into  units  of  r,  where  r,  the  radius  from  the  center  of  the  home 
range,  is  measured  in  the  a  units  of  the  bivariate  normal  distribution,  pro- 
vided the  constant  a.  oi  R  =  r/a  is  known.  This  conversion  was  arbitrarily 
accomplished  as  follows:  It  can  be  shown  that  the  cumulative  probability 
of  "capture"  of  Eq.  (23),  when  expressed  in  terms  of  a  and  r,  has  the  form : 

Cum  =  1  -  ^-—  (r2  +  2ar  +  2a2)e-'-/a  (25) 

2a:- 

By  Eq.  (24),  Curve  I,  when  r  =  1.2,  Cum  =  0.513.  Therefore,  by  succes- 
sive approximations,  utilizing  Eq.  (25),  it  was  found  that  when  a  =  0.44, 
Cumii  =  0.511  atr  =  1.2.  Therefore,  0.44  is  the  a  conversion  factor  applied 
to  ^  =  r/a,  so  that  Curves  I  and  II  may  be  compared  in  terms  of  the  bivariate 
normal  home  range  cr  distance. 

The  values  for  these  two  curves,  as  shown  in  Table  III,  are  shown  in 
Fig.  8.  Note  that  up  to  about  2<t  radius  these  two  curves  are  so  nearly 
identical  that  they  are  either  likely  to  approximate  actual  field  data  equally 
well.  There  is  considerably  more  "tail''  to  Curve  II,  but  since  so  few  ob- 
servations occur  in  the  greater  than  2a  range,  it  will  still  be  difficult  to 
decide  which  of  these  two  curves  most  nearly  approximates  actual  field 
data  for  the  longer  recapture  radii. 

However,  the  objective  was  to  determine  how  well  phenomena  observed 
in  the  use  of  one-dimensional  space  could  lead  to  a  curve  approximating 
the  bivariate  normal  distribution.  One  of  the  assumptions  was  that  the 
wandering  responsive  phase  was  proportional  to  radius  from  home.  In 
the  analysis  of  wanderings,  vacillations  at  ends  of  trips  in  the  one-dimen- 


22 


John  B.  Calhoun 


z 

s 
o 
W 

s 
o 


5   2 


tf 


a 

n 

■< 

H 

"■  s 

^    5 

^     « 

«  ^ 

^  pq 

H 

Pi    w 

6^ 

s 
p 
o 


+ 

+ 

+ 


0; 

0) 

> 

+ 

3 

^ 

^     + 

rH|N 


T3 

a; 

> 

u 

;^ 

3 

tn 

(-1 

Si 

S 

O 

O 

p:;  .2 


lOOCOOODOJt^OGCiCO-Ht^O  OOl 

o  d  o  o  d  o  o  d  c  o  o  c  c        d  d 


1^   fO   O   O   05 
'-lCCCOI>00OiO5O5O5O5O5O5Oi  005 


ooooooooooooo 


o  o 


lO  CO  o 

O    lO    iM    lO    QO    lO    05    O 
C^3    t^    t^    ^    ^1    CO    »C    CO    CO    O    O    t'-    CO 

COiOCO-— iOt^iCO-*0(X'C«DC2 
0^00    0    0(^00050205050505 


coocoooo 


o  o  o  o 


00   CO    o 
i—(l>.050502CO'^lO 

Ttiiocofoi-oo405cocoooioooo5 

'*COC0<-(I^O'30'tlt^00O5O505 
O^COIOC00000050505050505 


CO    rt< 
05    <0 

CO  00 


ooooooooooooo 


Tt<l:^.-i0505t^cOCOi-H-«tii-(0 
C^CO(MOOCO':t<COOOr}<^t^O 
iOcOCO'<*iOO(M05'tit^05050 
O'-HC0l0C0000005C50505O 

doodddd  oooo--! 


COC005^JiCOC^'*it^OCO«005  oo 

odd— lrHr-H(N'(M(MC0CO00CO  i-HC^ 


1.  The  Social  Use  of  Space 


23 


<  0.5 
m 


•  Ma/e 

O  Female  S  Fawns 


4.0" 


.3       .6       .9       1.2      1.5     1.8    2.1     2.4    2.7     3.0    3.3     3.6    3.9 
RADIUS,  r,  FROM  CENTER  OF  HOME  RANGE  IN  0"  UNITS 

Fig.  8.  Accumulated  probability  of  "capture."  Curve  /  is  the  expected  from  the 
bivariate  normal  distribution  function.  Curve  //  is  the  expected  based  upon  (a)  ter- 
mination of  trips  according  to  the  equation,  y  =  e«+''*  in  which  the  slope  h  is  negative, 
(b)  probability  of  v/andering  at  end  of  trips  is  proportional  to  radius,  and  (c)  captures 
only  occur  during  the  wandering  phase  at  end  of  trips.  Curve  ///  is  the  accumulated 
travel  path  with  no  wandering  at  end  of  trips  and  "captures"  represented  by  observa- 
tion of  the  animal  in  motion. 


sional  alley  shown  in  Fig.  7,  it  was  only  recorded  whether  or  not  a  wander- 
ing occurred  at  the  end  of  trips.  No  measure  was  made  of  the  length  of  the 
wandering.  However,  it  is  my  impression  that  the  longer  a  trip,  the  greater 
will  be  the  amount  of  wandering  at  the  end  of  a  trip  if  wandering  is  initiated. 
This  would  mean  that  the  amount  of  responsive  wandering  at  the  end  of 
trips  is  proportional  to  r-  rather  than  just  to  r.  If  this  is  really  the  case, 
then  we  can  derive  a  fourth  cumulative  probability  curve,  Curve  IV, 
which  has  the  form: 


Cumiv  =  1 


+  -  +  /2  +  1 


\e~^ 


(26) 


By  successive  approximations  utiUzing  Eq.  (25),  it  was  found  that  when 
a  =  0.32,  Cumiv  =  0.516  at  r  =  1.2.  Therefore,  0.32  is  the  a  conversion 
factor  apphed  to  R  =  r/a,  so  that  Curves  I  and  IV  may  be  compared  in 
terms  of  the  bivariate  normal  home  range  a  distance.  Note  that  thus  there 
is  forced  conformity  of  both  Cumn  and  Cumiv  to  Cumi  at  the  1.2  value 
of  r. 

Near  the  home  range  center.  Curve  IV  predicts  fewer  captures  than  is 
imphed  by  Curve  II  or  found  by  observed  captures.  Table  III.  However, 
at  the  longer  radial  distances  from  the  home  range  center,  Curve  W  more 


24  John  B.  Calhoun 

closely  approximates  Curve  I  and  the  observed  data  than  does  Curve  II. 
At  present  parsimony  demands  assuming  only  that  the  farther  an  animal 
moves  from  his  home  range  center,  the  more  likely  it  will  terminate  an 
outward  trip  and  respond  to  stimuli  near  the  place  of  stopping.  Extremely 
careful  observation  is  required  to  determine  if  animals  tend  to  wander  still 
farther  about  the  points  of  termination  of  trips  as  these  points  occur 
farther  from  home. 

B.  Travel-Path  Home  Range 

Captures  or  responses  represent  only  one  method  of  assessing  home 
range.  I  have  indicated  that  one  assumption  regarding  behavior  is  that  on 
the  outward  trip  from  home  and  on  the  return  trip  to  home,  the  animal  is 
in  a  preceptually  blind  state  during  which  static  stimuli  fail  to  elicit 
responses.  And  yet  it  is  possible  to  observe  and  record  the  presence  of  such 
nonresponsive  individuals  on  this  outward  and  return  trip.  Utilizing  Eq. 
(19)  and  considering  the  effect  of  the  geometry  factor  in  the  sense  that  the 
observer  in  a  two-dimensional  field  can  record  an  animal  only  if  it  passes 
directly  by  the  observer,  and  assuming  that  the  amount  of  wandering  at 
end  of  trips  is  minimal,  the  equation  for  the  cumulative  probability  of  ob- 
serving an  animal  during  its  travels.  Curve  III,  becomes: 

Cumin  =  1  -  (1  +  7?)e-«  (27) 

Data  for  Curve  III  with  the  a  constant  =  0.44  are  shown  in  Table  III  and 
on  Fig.  8.  Obviously,  if  "captures"  represent  such  observations,  the  animal 
will  appear  to  spend  more  time  closer  to  home.  Leopold  et  al.  (1951,  Fig.  50) 
provide  data  on  home  range  based  upon  visual  observation  of  marked  mule 
deer.  They  presented  their  data  in  terms  of  number  of  observations  within 
successive  100-yard  bands  from  the  site  of  capture.  The  actual  home  range 
center  will  be  on  the  a\'erage  somewhere  to  the  right  or  left  of  the  hne  of 
length  d  connecting  the  point  of  capture  and  the  point  of  later  observation, 
the  actual  radial  distance  from  the  true  home  range  center  would  be  K  •  d/2 
when  K  is  between  1 .0  and  1.414.  Without  going  into  the  origin  of  K,  it  is 
still  apparent  that  the  distances  given  by  Leopold  et  al.  (1951)  can  be 
utilized  as  approximating  proportionality  to  radial  distances  of  observa- 
tion from  the  home  range  center.  The  cumulative  probabilities  of  observa- 
tion for  102  observations  of  males  and  103  observations  of  does  and  fawns 
are  with  distance,  respectively:  100  yards  (0.363,0.495),  200  yards  (0.500, 
0.815),  300  yards  (0.678,  0.952),  400  yards  (0.726,  0.980),  500  yards 
(0.862,  1.0),  600  yards  (0.862,—),  700  yards  (0.932,  — ),  800  yards 
(1.0,  — ).  Noting  that  0.50  of  the  males  were  observed  within  200  yards 


J.  The  Social  Use  of  Space  25 

of  the  point  of  capture,  and  that  0.495  of  the  females  and  fawns  were  ob- 
served within  100  yards  of  the  point  of  capture,  and  utilizing  Eq.  (27)  and 
the  fact  that  R  =  r/a,  a,  in  the  sense  of  d,  for  males  is  270  yards  and  for 
females  and  fawns  is  137  yards.  On  this  basis  the  observed  cumulative 
probabilities  of  observation  were  converted  into  sigma  units  of  distance 
from  the  point  of  capture  and  posted  on  Fig.  8. 

There  is  forced  conformity  with  Curve  III,  Eq.  (27),  at  0.74(7.  However, 
the  further  proximity  of  the  observed  points,  up  through  l.oa,  to  Curve 
III  provides  credence  to  the  general  formulation  of  Eq.  (27).  Observa- 
tions may  occur  when  the  animal  is  on  either  the  outward  or  return  por- 
tions of  a  trip  or  during  the  wandering  at  the  end  of  a  trip.  The  more  an 
animal  wanders  at  ends  of  trips  relative  to  the  cumulative  travel  path, 
excluding  wandering  at  the  end  of  trips,  the  farther  the  observed  points 
may  be  expected  to  diverge  from  Curve  III  and  approach  Curves  I  and  II. 
In  fact,  were  it  possible  to  obtain  adequate  assessments  of  home  range  in 
terms  of  observations  made  of  the  individual  during  its  travels  as  well  as 
of  data  derived  from  responses  such  as  entering  traps,  it  would  be  possible 
to  calculate  wandering  distance  at  r. 


V.  Summary  of  the  Concept  of  Home  Range 

Developing  an  attachment  to  a  restricted  region  to  which  an  animal 
returns  after  wandering  outward  from  it  requires  that  the  individual  belong 
to  a  species  which  has  evolved  the  capacity  to  retain  the  memory  of  prior 
experience.  If  an  animal  remains  at  the  site  of  its  birth  there  is  no  question 
that  the  immediate  surroundings  of  this  site  will  be  known  better  than 
more  distant  ones.  However,  many  individuals  are  forced  from  the  site  of 
their  birth.  The  term  "forced"  is  here  used  as  a  postulate  derived  from  the 
observed  avoidance  of  fields  containing  strange  stimuli.  Parsimony  de- 
mands the  assumption  that  an  animal  will  remain  at  home  unless  forced 
away,  until  it  can  be  shown  that  an  animal  will  change  its  home  in  the 
absence  of  any  factors  which  might  tend  to  expel  it. 

Even  if  an  animal  is  expelled  from  the  site  of  its  prior  residence  and 
begins  to  wander  at  random,  it  will  be  chance  cover  some  areas  more  fre- 
quently than  others.  As  it  thus  becomes  familiar  with  a  restricted  area, 
it  will  increase  its  avoidance  of  less  frequented  areas.  This  process  will  lead 
to  a  repeated  frequenting  of  a  particular  restricted  location  which  can  be 
defined  as  "home."  Home  may  acquire  other  attributes,  such  as  the  con- 
struction of  a  den,  but  such  additional  attributes  are  not  necessary  for  a 
site  to  become  a  home. 

At  irregular,  probably  random,  intervals  the  individual  engages  in  ex- 


26  John  B.  Calhoun 

cursions  from  home.  During  these  outward  excursions  the  animal  remains 
in  a  refractory  state  during  which  it  is  unable  to  respond  to  stimuli  en- 
countered. Only  at  the  termination  of  trips,  including  the  period  of  wander- 
ing at  end  of  trips,  will  the  animal  be  able  to  respond  to  objects  or  situa- 
tions such  as  it  had  already  passed  on  the  outward  trip.  However,  those 
objects  which  were  bypassed  on  the  outward  trip  do  alter  the  probability 
of  terminating  trips.  The  greater  the  number  of  such  objects,  or  the  greater 
the  intensity  of  stimuli  emanating  from  them,  the  greater  will  be  the 
probability  of  a  trip  stopping  after  reaching  every  distance  from  home. 
Should  novel  stimuli  be  encountered  during  a  trip,  the  shorter  will  be  the 
interval  after  reaching  home  before  initiating  another  trip.  This  holds  in 
so  long  as  the  novel  stimuli  are  not  excessive.  In  this  case,  the  animal  will 
remain  at  home  for  unusually  long  periods  before  again  venturing  forth. 
The  probability  of  wandering  increases  with  distance  from  home.  These 
several  aspects  of  locomotion  and  responsiveness  to  stimuli  lead  to  a 
probabiUty  of  responding  within  the  region  about  an  animal's  home, 
which  so  closely  resembles  that  of  the  bivariate  normal  distribution  func- 
tion that  the  latter  may  be  used  to  represent  home  range. 


VI.  Continuous  Removal  Trapping  of  Small  Mammals 

When  a  large  number  of  snap-traps  are  set  within  a  habitat  where  mice 
and  shrews  live,  it  is  usually  observed  that  more  individuals  are  captured 
on  the  first  than  on  the  second  or  third  day,  and  that  fewer  animals  enter 
traps  on  the  third  than  the  second  day.  It  has  generally  been  assumed 
that  this  decline  in  catch  through  time  would  continue  were  the  traps  left 
set  for  a  longer  period.  Based  upon  this  assumption,  various  equations 
(e.g.,  Zippin,  1956;  Calhoun  and  Casby,  1958)  have  been  developed  for 
estimating  the  number  of  individuals  which  would  eventually  enter  the 
traps. 

Furthermore,  it  has  also  generally  been  assumed  that  the  catch  for  a  3- 
or  4-day  period  would  reflect  the  relative  species  composition  of  the  com- 
munity being  sampled.  This  assumption  served  as  the  basis  for  establishing 
the  North  American  Census  of  Small  Mammals  (Calhoun,  1949-1957). 
During  the  nine  years,  cooperators  provided  results  (see  NACSM  Release 
No.  9)  for  1615  standard  trap-lines  consisting  of  20  stations,  usually  50 
feet  between  stations,  3  traps  per  station,  run  for  three  consecutive  nights. 
Despite  the  laudable  objectives  of  this  cooperative  endeavor,  this  tre- 
mendous effort  has,  for  reasons  we  shall  shortly  see,  provided  inadequate 
data  for  insight  into  either  relative  density  or  species  composition. 

Questioning  the  usefulness  of  short-term  removal  trapping  developed 


1.  The  Social  Use  of  Space  27 

slowly.  It  began  in  1950  when  I  asked  A.  Dexter  Hinckley,  then  a  summer 
student  at  the  Jackson  Memorial  Laboratory,  to  continue  eight  NACSM 
lines  for  15  days  (see  Section  VI,  E  and  Fig.  13A).  Despite  the  expectation 
of  a  continually  declining  catch  after  the  first  3  days,  it  turned  out  that  the 
number  taken  from  day  4  onward  actually  increased  each  successive  day 
until  on  day  15  three  times  as  many  individuals  entered  traps  as  did  on 
day  1. 

These  results  initiated  an  intensive  effort  to  explore  the  results  obtained 
from  prolonging  the  period  of  removal  trapping.  I  am  particularly  indebted 
to  Drs.  William  L.  Webb  and  Earl  F.  Patric  of  the  Huntington  Wildlife 
Forest,  and  to  my  colleague.  Dr.  Kyle  R.  Barbehenn.  Without  their  per- 
mission to  utilize  here  some  of  the  data  from  their  extensive  studies  of 
removal  trapping,  it  would  have  been  impossible  to  develop  the  concepts 
elaborated  in  succeeding  sections  of  this  paper.  I  have  also  profited  from 
a  number  of  30-day  census  contributed  by  other  NACS]\I  cooperators. 

Our  concern  here  will  be  with  the  contribution  of  this  program,  and 
allied  research  developing  from  it,  to  furthering  our  understanding  of 
home  range  and  the  organization  of  small  mammal  communities.  The 
following  few  sections  merely  present  results.  Note  that  within  each  study 
presented,  the  several  species  present  markedly  different  patterns  of  cap- 
tm*e  by  traps  over  time.  Interpretations  based  upon  the  data  in  Section 
VI  are  given  in  Sections  VIII,  IX,  and  X. 


A.  Rich  Lake  Island,  New  York,  1952,  Sixty-Day  Removal  Study— Data 
Contributed  by  William  L,  Webb' 

Four  NACSM  traplines  were  run  on  this  island  in  the  Huntington 
Forest  for  60  consecutive  days,  beginning  August  16.  The  red-backed 
mouse,  Clethrionomys,  and  the  shrew  Sorex  formed  the  majority  of  the 
catch.  Figure  9  gives  the  results  for  the  first  30  days.  Note  that 
Clethrionomys,  which  entered  the  traps  in  large  numbers  during  the  first 
few  days,  was  by  the  end  of  30  days  represented  by  only  half  the  total 
catch  as  that  for  Sorex,  Avhose  peak  in  catch  per  day  did  not  come  until 
the  ninth  day.  Both  species  exhibited  a  secondary  minor  period  of  increase 
in  captures. 

During  the  same  summer,  34  NACSM  lines  were  run  for  the  standard 
3-day  period  in  similar  forests  on  the  mainland  (Table  IV).  As  on  the 
island,  comparatively  few  Sorex  entered  traps  on  mainland  areas  during 
the  first  3  days  of  trapping.  And  yet,  continuous  removal  trapping  for  an 
80-day  period  (Section  XI)  in  one  tract  of  the  mainland  forest  produced 
()17  Clethrionomys  and  1225  Sorex. 


28 


John  B.  Calhoun 


30-DAY 
TOTAL 
•  •  Clefhrionomys         97 

m- ■  Sorex 


10  16  20  25 

DAY  OF  TRAPPING 


Fig.  9.   Daily  removal  captures  of  the  two  most  abundant  species  inhabiting  a  30- 
acre  island;  recorded  by  Dr.  William  L.  Webb. 


Table  IV 

Comparison  of  Results  between  Short-Term  and  Long-Term  Removal 
Trapping  on  the  Huntington  Forest  during  1952 


Genus 


Location 


Clethrionomys  Sorex  Perornyscus 


80-Day    total    for    continuous    removal         617 
trapping  on  a  30-acre  mainland  area 
(see  Section  XI) 


1225 


77 


34  NACSM  lines  each  for  3  days  on  main-         753   (22 . 1 )  « 
land 


17   (0.5)         38   (1.1) 


4  NACSM  lines  on  Rich  Lake  Island 
60-day  total 
Total  1st  3  days 

133 
53   (13.3) 

243 

4   (1.0) 

27 
0 

Mean  3-day  totals  shown  in  parentheses. 


1.  The  Social  Use  of  Space 


29 


B.  Chadwick  Woods,  Montgomery  County,  Maryland,  Removal  Study, 
1958-1959— Data  Contributed  by  Kyle  R.  Barbehenn 

Five  circular  traplines,  each  with  a  radius  of  approximately  300  feet, 
were  used.  The  number  of  traps  per  station  and  the  interval  between 
stations  varied  among  the  lines.  However,  for  the  present  purposes  these 
differences  are  unimportant.  As  with  the  Rich  Lake  Island  study,  the 
genus,  here  Peromyscus,  with  the  initial  most  rapid  input  was  represented 
by  only  half  as  many  (57)  captures  as  another  genus,  Blarina,  which 
entered  the  traps  more  slowly  (116  taken)  (see  Fig.  10).  A  third  genus, 
Sorex,  began  its  period  of  maximum  captures  only  after  the  majority  of  the 
other  two  genera  had  been  removed,  but  only  48  were  captured.  Note  that 
Peromyscus  exhibited  a  fairly  rapid  rate  of  capture  until  approximately 
65%  of  the  30-day  total  had  been  taken.  Following  an  intervening  period 
with  very  few  captures,  there  ensued  a  secondary  period  of  increased 
captures. 

CHADWICK   WOODS- MONTGOMERY  COUNTY,  MARYLAND 


10  15  20 

DAY   OF    TRAPPING 


25 


30 


Fig.  10.  Differential  "schedules"  of  entering  traps  expressed  by  the  three  species 
recorded  by  Dr.  Kyle  Barbehenn  in  his  study  of  continuous  removal  trapping  in  an 
upland  hardwood  forest. 


C.  Comparative  Catches,  Huntington  Wildlife  Forest,  1952-1953— Data 
Contributed  bv  Earl  F.  Patric  and  William  L.  Webb 


During  these  two  years  Peromyscus  and  Blarina  were  universally  scarce 
on  the  Huntington  Forest.  In  order  to  determine  the  general  pattern  of 
their  input,  data  from  five  separate  plots  must  be  pooled. 


30 


John  B.  Calhoun 


Four  plots  consisted  of  concentric  circular  traplines,  75  feet  between 
circles,  with  one  trap  each  10  feet  along  the  lines.  Two  plots  consisted  of 
three  lines,  and  two  of  four  hues.  The  fifth  plot  is  represented  by  the  first 
15  days'  results  of  the  Rich  Lake  Island  study  presented  in  more  detail  in 
Section  VI,  B.  All  areas  were  sampled  for  at  least  15  days  (Fig.  11).  Each 
day  all  animals  captured  were  removed. 

Note  that  whereas  the  catch  of  Clethrionomys  initially  declines  through 


_^ 

Clethrionomys 
Sorex 

Peromyscus 
Blarina 

15 -DAY 

TOTAL 

286 

377 

70 

43 

" 

D 

--a 

5  10 

DAY   OF  CAPTURE 

Fig.  11.  Summated  results  from  five  continuous  removal  studies  conducted  by  Dr. 
Earl  Patric  and  Dr.  William  Webb  during  1952  and  1953  on  the  Huntington  Forest, 
New  York. 

time,  all  other  species  exhibit  an  increase  in  catch  per  day  for  the  first  4 
or  5  days. 


D.  Comparative  Catches,  Huntington  Wildlife  Forest,  1951 — Data  Con- 
tributed by  William  L.  Webb 


Nine  NACSM  lines  were  each  run  for  at  least  24  days  during  September 
of  1951  (Fig.  12).  In  contrast  to  the  following  two  years,  Peromyscus 
nearly  equaled  Clethrionomys  in  abundance.  Likewise,  Blarina  was  abun- 


1.   The  Social  Use  of  Space 

40 


31 


iCIethrionomys 


DAY  OF  CAPTURE 


Fig.  12.  A  continuous  removal  study  conducted  by  Dr.  William  L.  Webb  on  the 
Huntington  Forest  in  1951.  This  year  represented  a  time  when  the  dominance  relation- 
ship of  the  three  most  abundant  species  was  not  yet  clarified,  the  small  mammal  com- 
munity was  in  a  state  of  social  flux.  See  Table  V.  Points  shown  are  3-point  moving 
averages. 

dant.  Only  Sorex  was  rare.  The  relative  likelihood  of  capture  during  the 
initial  and  terminal  days  of  trapping  forms  a  most  interesting  series  (Table 
V) .  Initial  and  terminal  likelihood  of  capture  are  inversely  related  despite 
the  fact  that  usually  over  50%  of  each  genus  were  taken  during  the  middle 
period  of  trapping. 

Table  V 
Huntington  Forest  (1951)  24-Day  Continuous  Removal  Trapping 


Genus 


24-Day  total 


Proportion  of  24-day  total  during 


First  5  day.s 


Last  5  days 


Peromysctis 
Clethrionomys 
Blarina 
Sorex 


217 

267 

229 

41 


0.418 
0.311 
0.188 
0.024 


0.042 
0.079 
0.166 
0.585 


E.  Comparative  Catches  in  Maine  (1950)  and  Maryland  (1953) 

Eight  NACSM  lines  within  an  80-acre  tract  of  a  much  more  extensive 
continuous  forest  opposite  the  Hamilton  Station  of  the  Jackson  Memorial 


32 


John  B.  Calhoun 


Laboratory  on  Mt.  Desert  Island  formed  the  trapping  procedure  in  the 
Alaine  study.  Four  concentric  circular  traplines,  each  of  a  75-feet  greater 
radius  than  the  next  innermost  one,  having  one  trap  each  10  feet  along  the 
lines,  formed  the  trapping  procedure  in  the  Maryland  study.  During  the 
years  these  studies  were  conducted,  I  was  still  working  under  the  assump- 
tion (see  Calhoun  and  Webb,  1953)  that  the  large  numbers  of  animals 
taken  shortly  after  the  first  3  days  of  trapping  resulted  primarily  from  in- 
vasion from  beyond  the  periphery  of  the  trapping  area.  These  relatively 
short-term  removal  studies  are  presented  here  (Fig.  13)  since  they  provide 
further  insight  into  the  differential  rate  of  input  for  the  several  species 
making  up  the  small  mammal  community.  Total  lo-day  catch  by  genus 


B  (MARYLAND) 

^^ 

Peromyscus  ^ 

/7 

1            1  Blarina 

/ 

7/  / 

Pitymys 

]/  J 

1                       1 

DAY  OF  TRAPPING 


5  10  15 

DAY  OF    TRAPPING 


Fig.  13.  Two  short-term  removal  studies.  The  more  dominant  a  genus  happens  to 
be  the  sooner  will  50%  of  its  members  be  trapped.  In  Fig.  13.\:  B  =  Blarina;  P  = 
Peromyscus. 

for  the  Maine  study  (Fig.  loA) :  Clethrionomys  80,  Sorex  53,  Peromyscus 
75,  and  Blarina  132.  Total  14-day  catch  by  genus  for  the  Maryland  study 
(Fig.  13B) :  Peromyscus  45,  Blarina  76,  and  Pitymys  62.  The  Maine  study 
was  conducted  by  A.  Dexter  Hinckley  under  the  author's  direction,  and 
the  Maryland  study  was  conducted  by  the  author. 

F.  Comparative  Catches  of  Peromyscus  and  Clethrionomys 


Relationships  between  these  two  genera,  revealed  by  NACSM  census 
data,  have  been  particularly  helpful  in  developing  insight  concerning  com- 
munity organization.  Census  from  three  localities,  where  both  genera 
occur,  are  represented  by  a  large  number  of  traplines.  Those  run  in  each 


J.  The  Social  Use  of  Space 


33 


locality  encompass  a  period  of  several  years,  and  so  should  provide  a 
representative  picture  of  relationships  in  the  respective  habitats.  Results 
in  Table  VI  derive  from  37,080  "trap-days"  of  effort. 

Drs.  Earl  F.  Patric  and  William  L.  Webb  provided  the  New  York  data 
from  the  Huntington  Wildlife  Forest  at  Newcomb,  New  York.  The  majority 
of  the  animals  shown  were  taken  in  the  years  of  high  density,  1953-1954, 
although  the  few  taken  during  the  low  density  years  of  1955  and  1956  are 
included.  Dr.  J.  E.  Aloore  of  the  University  of  Alberta,  Edmonton,  pro- 

Table  VI 
Comparative  Catch  of  Two  Genera  in  Three  Localities 


Genus  Location     Number 

of  lines 


Day" 


Total,        Mean 

3  days  1-3    per  line 


New  York 


Clethrionomys  Alberta 


Maine 


90  586  470  380 

(0.408)      (0.327)      (0.265) 


1436 


36  117  103  83  303 

(0.390)      (0.338)      (0.272) 


SO 


90 


58 


36 


184 


(0.489)      (0.315)      (0.196) 


"  Proportion  of  3-day  2  shown  in  parentheses. 


16.0 


2.3 


New  York 

90 

93 

(0.274) 

117 
(0.344) 

130 
(0.382) 

340 

3.8 

Peromyscus      Alberta 

36 

241 

(0.577) 

122 

(0.292) 

55 

(0.132) 

418 

11.6 

Maine 

80 

132 

(0.434) 

103 

(0.399) 

69 

(0.227) 

304 

3.8 

vided  the  Alberta  census  including  the  years  1948-1956.  Dr.  John  A.  King 
and  two  U.  S.  National  Park  Rangers,  Clifford  Senne  and  L.  S.  Winsor, 
and  the  author  conducted  the  ]\Iaine  census  on  Alt.  Desert  Island  between 
1949  and  1952. 

Figure  14  shows  the  decline  in  catch  from  day  1  through  day  3  for  these 
two  genera  for  Maine  and  New  York. 

In  Maine  where  both  genera  have  low  densities,  their  respective  patterns 
of  decline  in  catch  through  time  are  very  similar.  However,  the  relatively 
greater  catch  of  Clethrionomys  on  day  1  should  be  noted  with  reference  to 


34 


John  B.  Calhoun 


the  trends  of  the  15-day  trap-out  as  shown  in  the  left-hand  graph  of  Fig. 
13  for  eight  NACSM  lines  also  run  on  Mt.  Desert  Island. 

In  New  York,  where  Peromyscus  had  the  same  density  as  in  Maine  but 
Clethrionomys  was  four  times  as  numerous  as  Peromyscus,  a  striking  differ- 
ence in  the  trends  resulted.  Clethrionomys  exhibited  a  typical,  though  some- 
what slow,  decline  through  the  three  consecutive  days.  In  contrast,  despite 
removal  trapping  and  thus  fewer  deer  mice  available  for  entering  traps,  a 
greater  number  of  Peromyscus  entered  traps  each  successive  day.  I  wish 
to  emphasize  that  competition  for  entering  traps  contributed  negligibly 
to  these  trends.  Three  times  as  many  traps  were  set  each  day  as  there  were 
total  animals  caught  for  the  entire  3-day  period. 

•  •    Clethrionomys 

A -A    Peromyscus 

■5(- 

B.    NEW  YORK 


DAY 


DAY 


Fig.  14.  Capture  rates  for  Clethrionomys  and  Peromyscus  in  Maine  and  New  York. 
Nearly  codominance,  or  lack  of  dominance,  is  reflected  by  the  Maine  data,  whereas  in 
New  York  Peromyscus  is  clearly  subordinate  to  Clethrionomys.  See  Table  VI. 

In  Alberta,  where  both  species  are  on  the  average  relatively  abundant 
(Table  VII),  there  exists  a  marked  seasonal  difference  (Fig.  15)  in  the 
trend  of  input  over  time  between  these  species.  During  the  spring,  when 
low  densities  characterize  both  species,  each  exhibits  a  rapid  rate  of  de- 
cline. In  contrast,  by  fall  when  high  densities  have  developed  for  both 
species,  Clethrionomys  shows  a  relatively  constant  input.  Note  the  reversal 
of  the  trends  of  input  for  Clethrionomys  and  Peromyscus  when  Fig.  1 1  and 
Fig.  15B  are  compared. 


VII.  Toward  a  General  Theory  of  Interspecific  and  Intraspecific  Use  of  Space 


The  data  presented  in  Section  VI  reveal  that  a  continuous  decline  in 
catch  from  one  day  to  the  next  during  removal  trapping  is  the  exception 


1.   The  Social  Use  of  Space 


35 


Table  VII 
Mean  Catch  per  Trapline  om  Dr.  Moore's  Alberta  Study  Area 


Season 

Spring 

Fall 

Clethrionomys 
Peromyscus 

5.00 
8.06 

11.90 
15.56 

2.18 
1.93 

rather  than  the  rule.  Even  where  the  numbers  taken  per  day  do  initially 
decline,  a  secondary  increase  usually  ensues  by  the  fifteenth  day  of  trapping. 
Furthermore,  many  species  actually  exhibit  an  increase  in  catch  per  day 
as  their  associates  are  removed.  Any  trend  of  increase  in  catch  after  previ- 
ous removal  of  associates  can  only  mean  that  the  survivors  have  in  some 
way  altered  their  behavior  so  as  to  increase  their  exposure  to  traps.  Further- 
more, we  must  conclude  that  those  caught  earlier  in  time  must,  while  still 
alive,  have  suppressed  this  change  of  behavior.  We  shall  now  consider  how 
these  results  make  possible  a  conceptualization  of  spatial,  temporal,  and 
social  organizations  of  the  small  mammal  community. 


A.   SPRING 

m  •  Clfthrionomys 

▲ A  Peromyscus 


ALBERTA  I, I-n-B  NACSM  LINES  1949-1957 

.7 


DAY 


B.  FALL 
i  ^  •  •  Clethrionomys 

N        A—  — -A  Peromyscus 

Y-        N 

\ 
\ 
\ 
\ 
\ 


DAY 


Fig.  15.  Seasonal  capture  rates  for  two  species.  With  the  increase  in  density  from 
spring  to  fall  (see  Table  VII)  Peromyscus  apparently  becomes  dominant  to  Clethrionomys. 


36  John  B.  Calhoun 

A.  A  Two-Species  System 

The  relationship  between  Clethrionomys  and  Sorex  on  the  Rich  Lake 
Island  (Section  VI,  A,  Fig.  9)  and  between  Peronujscus  and  Blarina  in 
the  Chadwick  Woods  study  (Section  VI,  B,  Fig.  10)  form  the  basic  data 
leading  to  this  formulation.  The  shrews — Sorex  and  Blarina,  respectively, — 
in  these  two  studies  exhibited  a  delay  in  entering  traps  until  many  of  the 
mice,  Clethrionomys  or  Peromyscus,  had  been  removed.  Yet  despite  this 
slowness  of  entering  traps,  twice  as  many  shrews  as  mice  were  taken  during 
the  30  days  of  trapping  in  each  study. 

These  data  pose  two  questions : 

1.  Why  were  there  twice  as  many  shrews  as  mice? 

2.  In  what  way  (and  why)  did  the  behavior  of  the  shrews  change  so 
that  after  se\'eral  days  of  trapping  they  were  more  exposed  to  traps 
than  initially? 

The  first  assumption  will  be  that  the  larger  the  home  range  the  greater 
the  likelihood  an  animal  will  encounter  a  trap  and  be  caught.  Since  during 
the  first  few  days  of  trapping  many  mice  but  few  shrews  were  taken,  despite 
the  greater  abundance  of  shrews,  the  mice  must  have  had  considerably 
larger  home  ranges  than  the  shrews.  Furthermore,  since  the  number  of 
shrews  taken  per  daj^  increased  during  the  first  10  days  of  trapping,  it 
follows  that  their  home  range  expanded  as  the  mice  were  killed  off.  This 
leads  to  the  conclusion  that  the  mice  in  some  way  inhibited  the  extent  of 
home  range  of  these  shrews.  We  may  now  designate  the  mice  as  being 
dominant  or  alpha  species  and  the  shrews  as  subordinate  or  beta  species. 

Formulation  of  a  theory  depicting  the  social  and  spatial  aspects  of  such 
a  two-species  system  recjuires  the  assumption  of  a  uniform  distribution  of 
centers  of  home  range  for  the  alpha  species.  This  represents  the  simplest 
assumption  leading  to  a  1:2  ratio  of  number  of  alpha  and  beta  species. 
In  the  preparation  of  Fig.  16,  a  field  of  uniformly  spaced  dots  (not  showTi 
in  Fig.  16)  was  plotted.  These  dots  represented  home  range  centers  for 
members  of  the  alpha  species.  A  circle  of  radius  half  the  distance  between 
centers  was  drawn  about  each  center.  Each  of  the  larger  circles  in  Fig.  16 
encompasses  some  portion  of  an  alpha  individual's  home  range. 

Now  w^e  can  ask :  Where  is  it  most  logical  to  find  the  home  range  centers 
of  beta  species?  They  should  be  located  at  points  minimizing  encounter 
by  members  of  the  beta  species  with  members  of  the  alpha  species.  The 
interstices  formed  by  juncture  of  each  set  of  three  neighboring  home  ranges 
of  alpha  species  represent  such  locations. 

Here  a  beta  individual  is  equidistant  from  three  alpha  individuals.  Dis- 
placement of  the  home  range  of  a  beta  species  member  from  such  a  point 


1.   The  Social  Use  of  Space  37 

will  increase  its  probability  of  encountering  at  least  one  member  of  the 
alpha  species.  About  each  such  home  range  center  a  smaller  circle  was 
drawn  (Fig.  16).  This  smaller  circle  represents  the  same  proportion  of  the 
beta  species  home  range  as  does  the  larger  circle  for  the  alpha  species. 

Examination  of  Fig.  16  shows  that  according  to  this  formulation  there 
will  be  in  an  ideal  steady  state  exactly  twice  as  many  individuals  of  the 
subordinate  beta  species  as  there  are  of  the  dominant  alpha  species.  This 
is  because  there  are  twice  as  many  interstices  between  uniformly  distributed 
alpha  home  ranges  as  there  are  alpha  home  ranges. 


Fig.  16.  Spatial  distribution  of  a  dominant  and  a  subordinate  species.  Large  circles 
represent  a  uniform  distribution  of  one  sigma  radius  portions  of  the  home  ranges  of  a 
dominant,  alpha,  species,  while  the  smaller  circles  represent  a  similar  proportion  of  the 
home  range  for  members  of  a  subordinate,  beta,  species.  See  text  for  other  details, 
Section  VII,  A. 

Now,  suppose  that  a  few  traps  are  placed  at  random  within  a  habitat 
characterized  by  inhabitation  by  such  an  alpha  and  a  beta  species.  The 
fifteen  dots  in  Fig.  16  represent  such  random  points.  It  is  readily  seen  that 
there  is  an  alpha  individual  exposed  to  nearly  every  trap,  but  few  in- 
dividuals of  the  beta  species  are  exposed  to  traps. 

Now  suppose  we  do  set  traps  in  such  a  system  and  remove  the  individuals 
caught.  It  is  the  general  experience  in  continuous  removal  trapping  that 
25-50%  of  the  members  of  the  alpha  species  which  are  taken  during  30 
days  are  actually  taken  during  the  first  3  days. 

This  can  only  mean  that  by  the  end  of  3  days  there  must  be  many  mem- 
bers of  the  beta  species  whose  neighbors  of  the  alpha  species  are  then  no 


38  John  B.  Calhoun 

longer  present.  In  the  absence  of  the  inhibiting  influences  emanating  from 
the  former  alpha  neighbors,  the  beta  individuals  then  make  a  comple- 
mentary expansion  of  their  home  range.  As  they  do  so,  some  beta  individuals 
come  into  contact  with  traps  and  are  also  removed.  Inspection  of  a  number 
of  continuous  removal  census  in  which  there  is  an  alpha  species  and  one  or 
more  subordinate  species  indicates  that  maximum  expansion  of  home  ranges 
of  the  subordinate  species  is  generally  reached  by  the  fifteenth  day  of 
trapping.  After  this  time,  the  catch  by  day  for  subordinate  species  also 
declines  over  time  since  fewer  and  fewer  remain  to  be  caught. 

B.  The  Nature  of  the  Inhibitory  Influence 

Both  Sorex  and  Blarina  in  Fig.  10  and  Sorex  in  Fig.  9  exhibit  an  increase 
in  catch  even  beginning  on  the  second  day  of  removal  trapping.  Similar 
results  apply  to  the  subordinate  species  included  in  Figs.  11-14,  although 
not  so  apparent  in  those  graphs  where  the  ordinate  represents  accumulated 
catch.  These  results  indicate  that  even  removal  of  a  small  proportion  of 
the  alpha  species  is  sufficient  to  induce  home  range  expansion  by  sub- 
ordinate species.  Thus,  the  means  of  communication  through  which  in- 
hibition operates  must  be  sufficiently  effective  and  repetitive  that  a  change 
in  the  general  field  intensity  (or  frequency)  of  stimuli  emanating  from  an 
alpha  species  is  detected  within  a  few  hours  at  least  by  subordinate  species. 

Bodily  contact  by  random  movement  is  unlikely  to  be  effective.  By  the 
same  token  that  it  takes  several  days  before  all  alpha  species  are  taken  in 
traps,  it  follows  that  in  many  instances  subordinate  species  would  be  un- 
aware of  the  absence  of  their  alpha  neighbors  if  this  detection  were  a  con- 
sequence of  a  change  in  frequency  of  contact.  Production  and  detection  of 
scent,  at  least  where  scent  signposts  are  concerned,  would  likely  operate 
to  inhibit  home  range  expansion  because  of  the  persistence  of  scent  beyond 
the  death  of  alpha  individuals.  Sight  is  unlikely  to  be  an  effectual  means 
of  detection  of  alpha  by  beta  species,  both  because  of  concentration  of 
activity  during  the  night  by  many  species  and  because  many  of  these  beta 
species  actuality  spend  considerable  time  under  the  leaf  mold. 

There  remains  vocalization  and  audition  as  the  means  of  communication. 
Although  there  is  as  yet  no  proof  that  such  is  the  means  of  communication 
whereby  individuals  can  detect  the  presence  of  unseen  neighbors,  it  stands 
out  as  the  most  likely  possibility.  iVIost  small  mammals  do  vocalize. 

Fewer  barriers  exist  that  might  prevent  or  distort  the  passage  of  sound 
through  the  environment  than  is  true  with  regard  to  light  stimuli  or  odors. 
In  the  following  discussions  vocalization  and  audition  will  be  assumed  to 
be  the  means  of  communication  within  and  between  species.  However, 


1.  The  Social  Use  of  Space  39 

identification  of  the  means  is  unessential  to  the  general  argument;  only- 
recognition  of  the  existence  of  some  effective  means  of  communication  is 
necessary. 


C.  The  Learning  of  Signals 

There  exists  the  possibility  that  the  response  of  one  individual  to  a 
signal  emitted  by  another  has  become  through  evolutionary  processes 
one  which  does  not  require  a  learned  association  between  the  signal  and 
some  act  on  the  part  of  the  emitter  for  its  development.  In  the  prior  history 
of  such  species  there  must  have  been  the  opportunity  for  associating  the 
signal  with  its  emitter  and  there  must  have  been  survival  value  in  the  re- 
ceptor developing  an  innate  response  to  detection  of  the  signal.  However, 
until  such  responses  to  signals  are  demonstrated  to  be  innate,  it  shall  be 
assumed  that  they  are  learned. 

We  may  now  ask,  "How  may  the  members  of  a  species  learn  a  signal 
when  the  individuals  are  characterized  by  fixed  home  ranges  which  may  be 
described  by  the  bivariate  normal  distribution  function?"  In  order  to  gain 
insight  into  this  question,  we  shall  consider  two  neighbors,  A  and  B.  A's 
home  range  center  is  fixed  whereas  B,  who  lives  some  distance  away, 
gradually  shifts  its  home  range  center  toward  that  of  A.  When  the  home 
range  centers  are  six  home  range  sigma  or  more  apart,  it  is  apparent  that 
the  probability  of  their  meeting  by  chance  will  be  extremely  remote.  This 
relative  probability  of  meeting  is  proportional  to  the  product  of  their 
density  functions  at  any  particular  point  (see  Table  2  in  Calhoun  and 
Casby,  1958). 

However,  as  the  home  range  center  of  B  approaches  that  of  A,  these 
two  individuals  will  meet  by  chance  on  very  rare  occasions.  Three  examples 
of  the  relative  probability  of  A  and  B  contacting  are  given  in  Fig.  17. 
When  the  home  range  centers  (HRC's)  are  3.9  sigma  apart,  one  peak  is 
1.5  sigma  from  ^'s  HRC  and  the  other  is  1.5  sigma  from  B's  HRC.  In 
examining  Fig.  17  it  is  well  to  keep  in  mind  that  we  are  considering  the 
probability  of  contact  at  points  along  the  line  connecting  the  two  home 
range  centers.  At  all  distances  intervening  between  home  range  centers, 
from  slightly  over  3  sigma  up  to  6  sigma,  there  are  always  two  peaks  in 
this  curve  of  probability  of  contact  between  two  neighbors.  As  the  home 
range  center  of  B  approaches  3  sigma  to  that  of  A ,  these  two  peaks  approach 
each  other  until  at  3  sigma  they  coincide  for  the  first  time.  This  single  peak 
of  highest  probability  of  contact  of  two  neighbors,  which  lies  exactly  half- 
way between  the  two  home  range  centers,  characterizes  all  distances  less 
than  3  sigma  intervening  between  the  home  range  centers. 


40 


John  B.  Calhoun 


Let  us  assume  that  when  .4  and  B  meet  there  is  some  interaction  between 
A  and  B.  That  is,  A  responds  to  B  and  B  responds  to  ^.  At  the  same  time, 
each  emits  a  signal.  If  such  chance  contact  occurs  frequently  enough  there 
exists  the  opportunity  of  each  individual  associating  the  other  with  the 
signal  emitted  at  the  time  of  interaction.  Furthermore,  we  may  assume  that 
learning  is  enhanced  by  other  factors  of  the  environment  being  constant 


CONCERNING    THE  LEARNING  OF  SIGNALS 


0.6  1.2  1.8  2.4 

er   DISTANCE  FROM  As  H.R.C. 


3.0 


Fig.  17.  Concerning  the  learning  of  signals.  Not  unless  home  range  centers  are 
3  sigma  or  less  apart  will  there  be  a  single  point  of  most  probable  contact  of  two  animals 
meeting  by  chance.  This  point  lies  halfway  between  the  line  connecting  the  two  home 
range  centers.  The  closer  home  range  centers  approach,  the  higher  will  be  the  proba- 
bility of  chance  contact  and  thus  the  more  likely  the  association  of  any  simultaneous 
signal  with  the  consequences  of  meeting. 


at  the  time  of  interaction.  When  home  range  centers  are  more  than  3 
sigma  apart,  the  two  points  of  greatest  probability  of  contact  are  separated 
from  each  other  and  therefore  are  unlikely  to  have  identical  surroundings. 
This  nonidentity  of  surroundings,  that  is  the  absence  of  identical  secondary 
reinforcers,  may  be  expected  to  retard  learning. 

However,  at  a  3-sigma  interval  between  home  range  centers,  there  is 
only  a  single  point  of  greatest  probability  of  contact;  thus,  at  this  inter- 
vening distance  between  home  range  centers  there  is  not  only  an  increased 


7.   Tfw  Social  Use  of  Space  41 

probability  of  contact  because  the  home  range  centers  are  closer  together, 
but  there  exists  a  greater  constancy  of  secondary  reinforcers  at  the  single 
point  of  greatest  probability  of  contact.  It  is  for  this  reason  that  I  suspect 
that  learning  of  signals  is  not  likely  to  be  effective  unless  home  range 
centers  are  3  sigma  or  less  apart. 

Once  an  animal  has  learned  to  associate  a  signal  with  the  animal  which 
emitted  it,  there  then  exists  the  opportunity  that  the  detecting  individual 
can  perceive  (hear)  the  signal  at  some  distance  from  the  emitter  and  make 
the  appropriate  response  of  approach  or  withdrawal.  How  far  the  signal 
may  be  detected  depends  upon  both  the  intensity  of  the  signal  emitted 
and  the  ability  of  the  detector  to  hear  it.  Presumably,  sensory  capacities 
for  detection  and  motor  capacities  for  emission  have  e\'olved  simultaneously 
and  in  harmony.  We  may  then  wonder  as  to  the  distance  over  which  such 
evolution  of  capacities  permits  the  detection  of  an  emitted  signal.  In  the 
absence  of  any  experimental  data,  introspection  suggests  that  one  might 
anticipate  evolution  of  capacities  to  the  point  that  an  individual  can  just 
detect  a  signal  emitted  at  the  maximum  distance  between  home  range 
centers  which  still  permits  the  learning  of  such  signals.  As  we  have  seen, 
this  distance  is  equivalent  to  3  home  range  sigma.  In  other  words,  when 
animal  A  is  at  its  home  range  center,  a  signal  emitted  by  animal  B  at  the 
border  of  A's  home  range  (as  represented  by  a  3-sigma  distance)  reaches 
A  in  just  the  sufficient  intensity  to  elicit  a  response  by  A.  As  B  moves 
farther  than  3  sigma  away  from  A,  the  signal  exhibits  further  decrease  in 
intensity.  Such  reduced  intensity  may  well  be  perceived  by  A  until  B 
gets  at  least  6  sigma  away.  Thus,  between  3  and  6  sigma,  it  is  suspected 
that  the  signal  itself  is  perceived  but  is  below  the  threshold  necessary  for 
eliciting  a  response  by  the  receptor.  Signals  arising  between  3  and  G  sigma 
from  the  receptor  are  here  designated  as  contributing  to  what  I  shall  call 
"hum." 

These  characteristics  of  the  signal  are  represented  schematically  in  Fig. 
18.  A  signal  emitted  by  one  animal  when  nearly  in  contact  with  another 
may  be  given  a  rating  of  1.0.  For  the  purpose  of  later  calculations,  it  is 
assumed  that  there  is  an  inverse  decrease  of  intensity  of  the  signal  at  suc- 
cessive distances  from  the  emitter  until  at  a  distance  of  3  sigma  it  has 
reached  one-tenth  of  the  intensity  that  might  be  recorded  at  the  emitter. 

Investigations  of  this  formulation  requires  that  the  sound  signals  emitted 
by  typical  individuals  of  a  species  be  recorded  and  other  individuals  of  the 
same  species  trained  to  exhibit  a  response  upon  perception  of  a  recorded 
vocalization.  Then,  in  the  native  habitat  of  the  species,  the  trained  subject 
must  be  moved  continually  farther  away  from  the  sound  source  until  it  no 
longer  exhibits  the  characteristic  response  which  it  had  been  trained  to 
perform  following  presentation  of  the  signal.  This  distance  may  then  be 


42 


John  B.  Calhoun 


compared  with  the  animars  home  range  sigma,  as  determined  by  Hve  trap- 
ping and  related  observational  procedures. 

D.  The  Distance  between  Neighbors  of  the  Same  Species 

From  the  observed  tendency  of  a  1:2  ratio  between  alpha  and  beta 
species  when  coexisting  in  the  same  habitat,  it  was  concluded  that  the 
home  range  centers  for  the  alpha  species  should  approximate  a  uniform 
distribution.  However,  this  analysis  provided  no  insight  into  whether  there 


^  0.04  - 

_j 

°^  (0-0.3O-) 

DISTANCE  FROM  H.R.C. 
AT  WHICH  SIGNAL  IS  PERCEIVED 

Fig.  18.  Theoretical  conceptualization  of  signal  characteristics  with  reference  to 
emission  at  a  constant  intensity  at  the  home  range  center  and  detection  and  response 
by  others  at  distances  from  it. 


might  be  some  ideal  distance  which  should  intervene  between  home  range 
centers.  In  order  to  seek  this  insight  we  shall  consider  a  single  species  com- 
munity in  which  all  individuals  have  exactly  the  same-sized  home  range, 
centers  of  home  range  are  uniformly  distributed,  and  the  bivariate  normal 
distribution  function  describes  the  home  range. 

Answers  to  two  questions  will  be  sought  with  reference  to  the  relative 
distance  between  home  range  centers: 

1.  How  does  distance  between  home  range  centers  affect  the  impact  of 
the  community  on  the  environment? 

2.  How  does  the  interval  between  home  range  centers  affect  the  proba- 
bility of  one  individual  meeting  or  detecting  its  neighbors? 

The  impact  of  an  individual  upon  its  environment  should  be  proportional 


J.  The  Social  Use  of  Space 


43 


to  the  amount  of  time  per  unit  area  it  spends  at  successive  distance  from 
its  home.  This  relative  impact  is  described  by  the  bivariate  normal  dis- 
tribution function  (Fig.  2).  For  any  particular  animal  this  means  that  at 
3  sigma  distance  from  its  home  its  impact  per  unit  area  will  be  only  0.011 
of  what  it  was  adjacent  to  its  home.  We  may  visualize  the  impact  of  any 
one  animal  upon  its  enviroimient  as  having  a  mountain-shaped  topography. 
Where  home  range  centers  are  at  least  6  sigma  apart,  there  lies  between 
them  a  "valley"  where  neither  animal  has  a  significant  effect  on  the  en- 


IMPACT    ON  THE   ENVIRONMENT 
(PER  UNIT  AREA) 


>- 
z 
<  o 

o  °^. 

I-  I 

Ul    to 

>  K 

-I  < 

UJ   UJ 

Q:  z 

i< 
"*  1 

>  z 

CD   < 

5 


0.4  I — 


-• /.5tr 


• • »        » 


-• •  2./<r 


3.0 


0  0.6  1.2  1.8  2.4 

o-  DISTANCE  FROM  ANY  H.R.C. 

Fig.  19.  Impact  on  the  environment  (per  unit  area).  The  value  1.0  represents  the 
effect  one  individual  will  have  near  its  home  range  center.  Since  home  ranges  increasingly 
overlap  as  their  centers  approach  each  other,  i.e.,  density  increases,  the  summated 
impact  of  all  animals  on  any  one  point  not  only  increases,  but  the  relative  impact  on 
all  points  becomes  more  nearly  equivalent. 


vironment.  As  soon  as  home  range  centers  get  closer  than  6  sigma  to  each 
other,  the  home  ranges  overlap  and  neighbors  can  both  affect  those  por- 
tions of  the  environment  falling  within  both  neighbor's  home  ranges.  As 
soon  as  home  range  centers  become  less  than  3  sigma  apart,  some  portion 
of  the  environment  can  be  affected  by  more  than  two  individuals. 

At  any  point  in  the  environment,  the  impact  of  all  animals  which  can 
arrive  at  that  point  during  this  normal  ranging  about  their  home  is  pro- 
portional to  the  sum  of  their  separate  density  functions  at  that  point. 
Utilizing  the  normative  data  of  density  function  as  a  function  of  the  sigma 
radius  from  home,  given  in  Table  2  of  Calhoun  and  Casby  (1958),  several 
curves  of  summated  density  function  were  calculated  (Fig.  19). 


44  John  B.  Calhoun 

E.  Methods    of    Calculating    Data    Relative    to    the    Distance    between 
Neighbors 

In  Fig.  16,  which  illustrates  the  uniform  distribution  of  an  alpha  species 
and  of  a  beta  species  lying  in  the  interstices  between  the  home  ranges  of 
the  alpha  individuals,  we  can  select  any  single  alpha  individual  and  note 
certain  characteristics  of  the  geometric  distribution  of  its  alpha  neighbors. 
One  such  individual,  whose  home  range  center  is  indicated  by  a  triangle, 
is  shown  in  Fig.  16.  A  line  drawn  between  the  home  range  centers  of  its 
adjoining  nearest  neighbors  forms  a  hexagon  about  this  individual.  Just 
as  there  are  six  nearest  neighbors,  there  are  twelve  next-nearest  neighbors. 
Lines  connecting  the  home  range  centers  of  these  next-nearest  neighbors 
also  form  a  hexagon.  Successively  more  distant  neighbors  form  concentric 
hexagons,  each  containing  six  more  indi\nduals  than  the  next  innermost 
hexagon.  For  the  purpose  of  investigating  the  effect  of  neighbors  on  each 
other  or  upon  the  environment,  a  system  of  four  "concentric"  hexagonal 
sets  of  neighbors  was  prepared  on  a  large  sheet  of  graph  paper.  This  pro- 
cedm'e  was  repeated  three  times,  forming  spatial  sets  in  which  home  range 
centers  between  nearest  neighbors  were  respectively  1.5,  2.1,  and  2.7 
sigma  apart.  Ruler  scales  representing  density  functions  (Table  2,  Calhoun 
and  Casby,  1958)  at  successive  sigma  distances  from  the  home  range 
center,  as  well  as  ruler  scales  representing  intensity  of  signal  (Fig.  18) 
were  prepared.  Using  these  ruler  scales,  several  types  of  events  were  calcu- 
lated with  regard  to  their  changes  in  intensity  or  freciuency  along  a  3- 
sigma  route  such  as  is  shown  by  the  heavy  dashed  line  in  Fig.  16. 

At  each  of  eleven  points  along  this  typical  route  of  travel,  a  sum  of  the 
density  functions  of  all  neighbors  whose  home  ranges  overlapped  one  or 
more  of  these  eleven  points  was  calculated  (see  Fig.  19). 

F.  Further  Comment  on  the  Impact  of  All  Individuals  on  the  Environment 

Each  of  these  sums  of  density  functions  were  divided  by  0.159,  the  rela- 
tive density  function  of  an  animal  near  its  own  home  range  center.  By  so 
doing  we  can  obtain  a  fairly  good  idea  of  the  impact  of  all  individuals  who 
may  arrive  at  any  particular  point  with  reference  to  the  effect  that  one 
individual  would  have  near  its  home  range  center.  It  may  be  seen  that 
when  home  range  centers  are  2.7  sigma  apart,  considerable  inequality 
between  points  exists.  In  other  words,  points  near  home  range  centers  are 
relatively  intensively  used  in  comparison  to  distances  about  halfway 
between  home  range  centers.  This  inequality  of  usage  of  the  environment 
is  even  more  pronounced  when  home  range  centers  are  more  than  2.7 


1.  The  Social  Use  of  Space  45 

sigma  apart.  However,  by  the  time  home  range  centers  are  uniformly  dis- 
tributed at  2.1  sigma  apart,  all  portions  of  the  environment  are  approxi- 
mately equally  utilized  although  every  point  is  more  intensively  utilized 
than  when  home  range  centers  were  farther  apart.  Every  further  increase 
in  density,  as  represented  by  home  range  centers  coming  closer  together, 
merely  increases  the  intensity  of  usage  of  every  part  of  the  environment  and 
all  parts  continue  to  be  equally  utilized. 

These  curves  (Fig.  19)  are  particularly  instructive  in  gaining  an  insight 
into  an  "ideal"  interval  between  home  range  centers.  It  is  logical  to  assume 
that  portions  of  the  environment  which  are  less  utilized  than  others  serve 
as  a  trap  to  catch  wandering  individuals  who  have  not  yet  established  a 
home  range.  As  long  as  the  process  of  eciualizing  distance  between  adjoining 
home  range  centers  continues,  no  remaining  pockets  of  less  utilized  habitat 
will  occur  by  the  time  adjoining  home  range  centers  are  nearly  2.1  sigma 
apart. 

Any  increase  in  density,  that  is  any  shortening  of  the  interval  between 
home  range  centers  below  2.1  sigma,  will  merely  increase  the  probability 
that  available  objects  will  be  overutilized. 

At  the  maximum  interval  between  HRC's  at  which  uniform  utilization 
of  the  environment  arises,  aggressi^'e  actions  exhibited  by  individuals  with 
resident  home  ranges  may  be  expected  to  prevent  excess  members  of  the 
populations  from  settling  down  within  such  an  established  area.  Such 
wandering  individuals  may  be  expected  to  wander  through  and  out  of  such 
established  areas  and  into  marginal  habitats. 

Thus,  if  minimizing  the  opportunity  for  aggressive  encounters  and  the 
development  of  a  uniform  utilization  of  resources  represent  forces  affecting 
evolution,  we  may  anticipate  development  of  capacities  for  communication 
which  will  most  readily  assure  that  the  members  of  a  population  of  a  single 
species  will  be  able  to  distribute  themselves  uniformly  through  space  with 
an  approximate  2.0  home  range  sigma  distance  intervening  between  any 
two  adjoining  home  range  centers. 

G.  Contacting  Neighbors 

The  product  of  the  density  functions  of  any  two  individuals  at  a  particu- 
lar point  determines  the  relative  probability  that  these  individuals  will 
meet  by  chance.  Similarly,  the  product  of  the  density  function  of  any  one 
individual  at  a  point  with  the  sum  of  the  density  functions  of  all  other 
individuals  determines  the  relative  probability  that  this  one  individual 
will  contact  neighbors  at  that  point.  Such  latter  calculations  were  made 
for  uniformly  distributed  home  range  centers  at  2.7,  2.1,  and  1.5  sigma 


46 


John  B.  Calhoun 


(see  Fig.  20) .  As  might  be  expected,  as  home  range  centers  get  closer  to- 
gether the  probability  of  any  individual  contacting  neighbors  at  every 
distance  from  its  home  range  center  increases.  There  may  be  some  fre- 
quency of  contacting  neighbors  which  becomes  so  unbearable  to  the  in- 
dividual that  his  resultant  aggressive  actions  prevent  further  contraction 
of  the  interval  between  home  range  centers.  However,  we  have  no  basis 
for  gaining  insight  as  to  what  this  frequency  might  be. 


0.1  r- 


•-  -^     .01 


L.    < 

O   t- 


-I  q: 

m  "^ 

o 

tc 

a. 


.001 


.0002  ■- 


0.6  1.2  1.8  2.4  3.0 

<r  DISTANCE  FROM  ANY  H.R.C. 


Fig.  20.  Contacting  neighbors.  The  relative  probability  of  any  one  individual  meet- 
ing others  with  reference  to  its  distance  from  its  own  home  range  center,  and  to  the 
distance  between  home  range  centers  of  all  individuals. 


One  characteristic  of  these  curves  of  relative  probability  of  contacting 
neighbors  does  lend  itself  to  suggesting  a  condition  leading  to  an  optimum 
interval  between  home  range  centers.  When  HRC's  are  2.1  sigma  apart 
the  probability  of  contacting  neighbors  up  to  half  the  distance  between 
home  range  centers  is  for  all  practical  purposes  constant.  Applying  this 
insight  to  all  members  of  the  population,  it  is  apparent  that  with  HRC's 
this  distance  apart,  the  probability  of  contact  between  neighbors  becomes 
relatively  constant  everywhere.  If  we  accept  the  principle  elaborated  by 


1.  The  Social  Use  of  Space  47 

Fredericson  (1951)  that  animals  attempt  to  make  their  environment 
predictable,  and  if  we  accept  constancy  of  consequences  as  assuring  greater 
predictability,  then  it  follows  that  where  the  members  of  a  community 
have  their  home  range  centers  approximately  2.1  sigma  apart,  greatest 
predictability  with  regard  to  contacting  neighbors  characterizes  this 
interval. 


H.  Sign  Field  of  AlljNeighbors 

Urination,  defecation,  and  activities  relating  to  the  removal  of  materials 
used  for  food  or  nests  represent  signs  by  which  one  individual  might  recog- 
nize the  presence  of  neighbors.  Signs  left  by  neighbors  may  be  expected  to 
be  proportional  to  the  sum  of  the  density  functions  of  neighbors  at  points 
considered.  Unless  home  range  centers  are  extremely  close  together,  there 
will  arise  a  noticeable  increase  in  signs  of  neighbors  as  the  individual  moves 
away  from  its  own  home  range  center.  Obviously,  the  closer  home  range 
centers  are  to  each  other  the  relatively  greater  will  be  the  sign  of  neighbors 
at  any  particular  radius  from  the  individuals's  own  home  range  center. 
We  may  then  wonder  what  standard  a  particular  individual  may  utilize 
in  judging  the  intensity  of  signs  left  by  its  neighbors.  Any  individual's 
own  sign  is  maximal  near  its  own  home  range  center.  Therefore,  an  in- 
dividual may  resort  to  comparing  the  relative  amount  of  sign  of  neighbors 
at  any  point  to  that  which  it  would  leave  in  a  similar  area  near  its  own  home 
range  center. 

Dividing  the  sum  of  the  density  functions  of  all  neighbors  at  a  particular 
point  by  the  density  function  of  a  particular  individual  near  its  home  range 
center  provides  such  an  index  of  the  relative  intensity  of  sign  of  neighbors 
(Fig.  21).  The  optimum  interval  between  home  range  centers  with  regard 
to  the  sign  field  should  be  that  interval  at  which  throughout  the  home  range 
of  a  particular  individual  the  total  sign  left  by  neighbors  nearest  approxi- 
mates that  individual's  own  standard  and  in  which  there  is  greatest  pre- 
dictability with  regard  to  sign,  that  is  in  which  there  is  the  least  variation 
in  intensity  of  neighbors'  signs  from  point  to  point.  Judging  from  the  three 
curves  presented  in  Fig.  21  an  inter-home  range  center  interval  of  some- 
where near  2.1  sigma  would  lead  to  the  development  of  an  optimum  sign 
field. 

I.  Signal  Field  of  Neighbors 

Following  the  formulation  presented  in  Section  VII,  C,  it  is  assumed 
that  the  signal  emitted  by  one  individual  can  be  perceived  by  another  in- 


48 


John  B.  Calhoun 


dividual  with  sufficient  intensity  to  produce  a  response  by  the  latter  in  so 
long  as  the  indiA'iduals  are  separated  by  a  distance  no  greater  than  3  home 
range  sigma.  In  all  probability,  signals  in  the  sense  of  vocalizations  are 
emitted  by  each  individual  periodically  as  they  wander  through  their 
home  range.  In  order  to  simplify  calculation  of  the  signal  field  of  neighbors, 
the  particular  condition  was  taken  where  all  signals  are  emitted  only  from 
the  home  range  centers.  Thus,  along  a  typical  route  of  travel,  as  shown  by 
the  heavy  dashed  line  in  Fig.  1(),  the  sum  of  the  intensity  of  signals  from 
all  neighbors  was  calculated. 


0.6  1.2  1.8  2.4 

a  DISTANCE   FROM  ANY  H.R.C. 


Fig.  21.  Sign  field  of  neighbors.  Signs  are  considered  as  any  persisting  indication  of 
an  animal  having  made  a  response,  i.e.,  defection,  gnawings,  or  removal  of  food  items. 
Thus  Fig.  21  essentially  represents  the  subtraction  of  the  density  function  of  one  indi- 
vidual from  the  sum  of  the  density  functions  of  all  individuals  as  shown  in  Fig.  19. 


Again,  we  might  wonder  what  standard  the  individual  might  utilize  in 
judging  the  total  intensity  of  signals  received.  Since  the  learning  of  the 
signal  presupposes  emission  by  one  individual  and  detection  by  the  other 
when  they  are  in  contact,  this  level  of  intensity  with  an  assigned  value  of 
1.0  may  be  taken  as  the  standard.  Since  the  intensity  of  signals  probably 
drops  off  inversely  proportional  to  distance,  the  sum  of  signals  at  any 
point  in  place  and  time  may  be  less  than  1.0.  A  further  complication  to  the 
problem  is  that  all  neighbors  may  not  emit  signals  simultaneously.  Simul- 
taneity other  than  by  chance  will  arise  regularly  only  if  the  detection  of  the 


1.  The  Social  Use  of  Space 


49 


signal  by  one  individual  elicits  a  similar  response  by  the  perceiver.  However, 
if  this  is  so,  and  if  each  individual  after  emitting  a  burst  of  sequential 
signals  enters  a  refractory  period  (see  Section  XIII,  A)  of  some  given 
mean  length  before  it  can  emit  signals  again,  then  we  have  a  situation  in 
which  there  occur  recurrent  periods  during  which  most  nonsleeping  in- 
dividuals in  the  community  emit  signals  nearly  simultaneously.  Although 
no  proof  of  the  validity  of  this  assumption  can  be  offered  at  present,  my 
formulation  will  accept  the  existence  of  such  a  process.  Such  an  assumption 
is  inherent  in  the  utilization  of  P'ig.  22  in  arriving  at  some  insight  as  to  the 


2.0 


1.0 


0.3  >— 


/.5ff- 


0.6  1.2  1.8  2.4 

o-  DISTANCE  FROM  ANY  H.R.C. 


3.0 


Fig.  22.  Signal  field  produced  by  neighbors.  If  all  neighbors  emit  signals  simul- 
taneously at  their  home  range  centers,  and  these  signals  have  the  properties  shown  in 
Fig.  IS,  then  their  summated  intensity  will  form  a  "topography"  as  here  shown  with 
reference  to  any  particular  animal  moving  through  its  home  range.  The  value  1.0  repre- 
sents the  intensity  of  a  .signal  at  the  point  of  emission. 


influence  of  the  signal  field  on  determination  of  an  optimum  interval  be- 
tween home  range  centers. 

As  with  the  sign  field  of  neighbors,  the  signal  field  of  neighbors  is  pre- 
sumed to  influence  interval  between  home  range  centers  through  the  mem- 
bers of  the  community  seeking  that  interval  between  home  range  centers 
which  will  ensure  most  closely  attainment  of  both  constancy  of  the  signal 
field,  leading  to  predictability  and  to  approximation  of  the  standard  signal 
intensity.  Judging  by  the  three  curves  in  Fig.  22,  operation  of  these  two 
criteria  indicates  an  optimum  interval  between  home  range  centers  slightly 
less  than  2.1  sigma. 


50 


John  B.  Calhoun 


J.  Hum  Field 

In  Section  VII,  C  it  was  suggested  that  when  the  emitter  is  between  3 
and  6  sigma  from  the  receptor  the  signal  given  by  the  emitter  can  be  per- 
ceived by  the  receptor  but  is  insufficient  in  strength  to  elicit  the  appropriate 
responses.  The  sum  of  all  such  signals  below  threshold  for  inducing  a 
response  is  here  termed  "hum."  When  this  value  exceeds  the  standard 
intensity  of  1.0  (see  Section  VII,  I),  the  receptor  will  become  restless  even 
though  perhaps  not  exhibiting  a  specific  response  to  the  signals.  What 
effect  this  general  state  of  restlessness  may  have  upon  the  receptor  is  un- 
known, but  it  is  logical  to  assume  that  the  members  of  the  community 


3.0  I —    •_ 


>   20 

in 


UJ 

>     1.0 

t- 
< 

_l 

LiJ 


0.4 


_• ■ a- 


/.5  a- 


2.IO- 


27a- 

\ 
Distance 

between 

H.  R.  C.  's 


0.6  1.2  1.8  2.4 

a  DISTANCE   FROM   ANY  H.R.C. 


30 


Fig.  23.  Hum  field  refers  to  the  sum  of  the  intensity  of  all  signals,  any  one  of  which 
is  below  that  minimum  intensity  required  to  elicit  a  specific  response.  The  value,  1.0, 
denotes  the  intensity  of  a  signal  at  the  point  of  emission. 

will  attempt  to  adjust  the  interval  between  the  home  range  centers  in  such 
a  way  as  to  reduce  the  likelihood  of  the  "hum"  exceeding  1.0.  To  do  so 
implies  that  home  range  centers  must  be  of  the  order  of  2.4  sigma  apart 
(Fig.  23). 

K.  General  Conclusion  Concerning  the  Distance  between  Neighbors 


Effective  learning  of  signals  probably  does  not  begin  until  home  range 
centers  come  at  least  3.0  sigma  from  each  other,  and  even  further  shorten- 
ing of  this  interval  must  increase  the  effectiveness  of  learning.  Several 


1.  The  Social  Use  of  Space  51 

factors  examined  all  suggest  that  the  optimum  interval  between  home 
range  centers  should  be  of  the  order  of  a  2  home  range  sigma  distance. 
From  an  evolutionary  standpoint,  the  most  important  of  these  probably  is 
the  fact  that  utilization  of  the  environment  becomes  uniform  at  slightly 
more  than  2  sigma  distance  between  home  range  centers.  This  factor  should 
have  been  the  major  one  in  the  evolution  of  those  characteristics  of  in- 
dividuals pertaining  to  the  frequency  of  trips,  the  velocity  of  the  individuals, 
and  the  aggressive  acts  elicited  when  two  individuals  meet.  It  is  probably 
strictly  coincidental  that  the  properties  of  the  sign  and  signal  field  are  also 
likely  to  be  such  as  to  make  an  interval  of  about  2  sigma  between  home 
range  centers  optimum.  It  is  at  this  distance  between  home  range  centers 
that  both  signs  and  signals  become  most  constant  and  thus  more  pre- 
dictable. Also,  the  intensity  of  the  signs  and  signals  most  nearly  approxi- 
mate any  receptor's  own  behavior  near  its  home,  which  can  serve  for  it  as  a 
standard  in  evaluating  the  intensity  of  the  actions  of  its  neighbors  impinging 
on  it. 


L.  The  Number  of  Neighbors  Perceived 

When  home  range  centers  are  2.7  sigma  apart,  an  indi\ddual  at  its  home 
range  center  can  just  perceive  all  its  six  nearest  neighbors.  However,  as  it 
begins  to  move  away  from  home  it  begins  to  lose  contact  with  those  nearest 
neighbors  lying  on  the  opposite  side  of  its  home  range  center.  By  the  time 
it  reaches  half  the  distance  to  its  nearest  neighbors  in  the  direction  in 
which  it  is  traveling,  it  can  detect  only  three  of  its  nearest  neighbors. 
Toward  the  periphery  of  its  home  range,  it  can  detect  only  two  of  its  nearest 
neighbors.  In  addition,  it  can  detect  one,  and  only  one,  of  its  next-nearest 
neighbors,  w^hich  are  members  of  the  group  of  twelve  forming  the  second 
hexagonal  tier  of  neighbors  about  its  home  range  center.  Thus,  with  this 
fairly  large  interval  between  home  range  centers,  any  one  individual  has 
poor  contact  with  its  associates.  When  home  range  centers  are  2.1  sigma 
apart,  any  one  individual  can  maintain  contact  with  all  six  of  its  nearest 
neighbors  out  to  about  0.7.")  sigma.  Even  when  an  individual  has  journeyed 
halfway  in  a  direction  of  a  nearest  neighbor  it  is  still  in  contact  with  four 
of  its  six  nearest  neighbors,  in  addition  to  one  of  the  twelve  neighbors 
lying  in  the  next  tier.  As  he  proceeds  still  farther,  losses  of  nearest  neighbors 
are  compensated  for  by  next-nearest  neighbors.  Thus,  at  all  times  when 
home  range  centers  are  2.1  sigma  apart,  an  individual  is  in  contact  or  po- 
tential contact  with  five  or  six  of  its  neighbors. 

By  the  time  home  range  centers  are  of  l.o  sigma  apart,  an  individual  is 
in  potential  contact  with  ten  other  indi\'iduals  though  not  always  the  same 


52  John  B.  Calhoun 

ten,  no  matter  where  it  is  in  its  home  range.  Since  the  dynamics  of  the  use 
of  space  relating  to  uniformity  of  utihzation  of  resources  and  the  character- 
istics of  the  sign  and  signal  field  all  point  to  an  optimum  interval  between 
home  range  centers  of  somewhere  near  2  sigma,  it  follows  that  there  should 
have  been  evolutionary  adjustment  of  tolerance  to  simultaneous  or  near 
simultaneous  communication  with  five  to  ten  others. 


VIII.  Interpretations  of  Observed  Data  Derived  from  Removal  Trapping  of 

Small  Mammals 

At  this  stage  in  the  development  of  a  concept  of  community  organiza- 
tion, one  must  resort  to  a  certain  amount  of  quasi  circular  reasoning. 
Regularities  in  observed  results  lead  to  theoretical  formulations.  Then  these 
formulations  can  be  used  to  reexamine  the  data  for  further  insight.  This  is 
my  present  intent.  In  time,  many  aspects  of  the  concept  may  be  subjected 
to  more  rigorous  study.  However,  for  the  present  we  must  content  ourselves 
with  a  search  for  a  best  approximation  to  a  very  complex  set  of  phenomena. 

Section  VI,  "Continuous  Removal  Trapping  of  Small  Mammals," 
presented  results  from  several  extensive  studies.  Specific  interpretations 
follow\ 


A.  The  Relationship  between  Two  Dominant  Species 

The  dominant  species  in  the  community  reveals  itself  during  removal 
trapping  through  its  members  having  such  large  home  ranges  that  every 
individual  living  near  a  trap  has  a  high  probability  of  encountering  it. 
Thus,  for  them,  few^r  days  lapse  from  initiation  of  trapping  until  50%  of 
the  population  is  caught.  As  can  be  seen  from  the  two  Alaryland  studies 
presented  (Fig.  10  and  Fig.  13B),  Peromyscus  fulfills  this  criterion.  For 
species  associated  with  Peromyscus,  whether  they  be  Blarina  and  Sorex,  or 
Blarina  and  Pitymys,  the  dates  of  509('  removal  arrive  much  later.  The 
later  the  date  of  50%  removal,  the  more  subordinate  a  species,  and  the 
more  slowly  its  members  expand  their  home  ranges  as  the  dominant 
species  is  removed. 

In  more  northern  forest  habitats,  Peromyscus  rarely  is  found  in  the 
absence  of  Clethrionomys.  In  fact,  it  is  as  if  the  red-backed  mouse  is  just 
superimposed  upon  the  simpler  Peromyscus-Blarina- Sorex  community  of 
more  southern  forests.  Typical  dominance  of  Clethrionomys  over  Peromyscus 
may  be  seen  in  Figs.  9,  11,  and  13A.  Although  I  am  convinced  that 
Clethrionomys  usually  can  develop  the  ability  to  inhibit  the  home  range  of 


1.  The  Social  Use  of  Space  53 

Peromyscus,  there  exist  conditions  limiting  the  extent  to  which  this  abihty 
may  develop. 

The  typically  low  relative  density  of  both  species  (Table  VI)  on  Alt. 
Desert  Island,  Maine,  represents  such  a  condition.  As  revealed  in  Fig.  14, 
both  species  decline  at  about  the  same  rate  from  day  1  through  day  3. 
This  can  happen  only  when  no  alteration  in  home  range  size  transpu-es 
over  time  or  when  the  survivors  of  each  species  make  equivalent  but  slight 
increases  in  extent  of  home  range.  Appreciation  of  why  inhibition  of  home 
range  fails  to  develop  at  low  densities  demands  knowledge  of  variables  we 
lack. 

It  demands  that  we  know  actual  densities.  The  NACSAI  census  provide 
only  relative  densities.  However,  we  can  make  approximations.  Run  long 
enough  (30  days),  the  950-foot-long  B-type  NACSM  census  procedure 
should  take  all  residents  wdthin  3  home  range  sigma  on  either  side  of  the 
trapline  and  for  a  radius  of  this  distance  about  the  end  of  the  line.  If  we 
take  50  feet  as  approximating  the  average  home  range  sigma  of  small 
mammals,  uninhibited  by  dominants,  then  approximately  8  acres  are  ex- 
posed to  such  a  trapline.  Furthermore,  examination  of  30-day  censuses  re- 
veals that  for  species  with  uninhibited  home  ranges,  25-50%  of  the 
residents  are  taken  during  the  first  3  days  of  trapping.  On  this  basis,  there 
was  on  the  average  less  than  one  Clethrionomys  and  less  than  two  Peromys- 
cus per  acre  in  this  Maine  study.  Since  juveniles,  with  as  yet  probably  little 
influence  on  the  spatial  distribution  of  associates,  comprised  a  portion  of 
the  catch,  it  is  quite  likely  that  the  average  distance  between  home  range 
centers  for  each  species  exceeded  3  sigma.  As  shown  in  the  prior  theoretical 
sections,  learning  of  signals  would  most  likely  be  fairly  ineffective  here 
because  contact  between  neighbors  w^ould  be  infrequent. 

Under  these  circumstances  the  signals  emitted  by  each  species  should 
have  acquired  little  in  the  way  of  negatively  stimulating  characteristics 
for  its  own  members.  It  follows  that  inhibition  of  home  range  size  will 
have  been  negligible  and  thus  the  two  species,  which  probably  have  nearly 
the  same  size  home  range,  should  encounter  traps  with  nearly  equal  fre- 
quency, and  thus  the  rate  of  decline  in  catch  from  days  1  through  3  should 
be  nearly  equal.  However,  in  the  one  Mt.  Desert  Island,  Maine,  study  where 
eight  NACSM  lines  were  run  for  15  days  (Fig.  13A),  it  is  apparent  that 
home  ranges  of  Peromyscus  were  slightly  contracted.  Fifty  per  cent  of  the 
15-day  total  for  Clethrionomys  was  attained  by  day  6,  but  not  until  day  10 
for  Peromyscus.  Thus,  where  both  species  regularly  occur  at  low  densities, 
Peromyscus  is  only  moderately  subordinate  to  Clethrionomys. 

In  the  Adirondacks,  where  Peromyscus  most  frequently  has  a  low  density 
and  Clethrionomys  a  much  higher  one  (Table  VI),  Peromyscus  is  markedly 
subordinate.  Its  home  ranges  not  only  are  markedly  contracted,  but  also 


54  John  B.  Calhoun 

they  expand  immediately  as  Cleihrionumys  are  removed.  The  vahdity  of 
this  interpretation  is  revealed  in  Fig.  14B,  which  shows  that  during  the 
first  3  days  of  trapping  as  the  Clethrionomys  population  is  reduced,  as  in- 
dicated by  fewer  numbers  taken  on  successive  days,  the  numbers  of  Pero- 
myscus  taken  increases.  This  increasing  catch  can  only  result  from  sufficient 
expansion  of  home  range  by  survivors  to  bring  about  an  increased  prob- 
ability of  encountering  traps. 

One  might  argue  that  these  latter  data  for  Pewmyscus  merely  indicate 
that  the  snap-trap  is  initially  a  sufficiently  strange  object  to  elicit  avoidance. 
As  time  elapses  these  mice  become  accustomed  to  the  presence  of  the  trap 
and  thus  later  in  time  more  individuals  will  enter  traps.  Two  lines  of  evi- 
dence of  reasoning  suggest  the  fallacy  of  this  interpretation.  If  it  were  cor- 
rect, we  must  conclude  that  Pewmyscus  in  Maine  lack  this  strange  object 
response  but  those  in  New  York  have  it  highly  developed  (see  Fig.  14). 
There  exists  no  logical  basis  for  believing  that  such  a  difference  character- 
izes the  populations  of  these  two  areas.  Furthermore,  in  the  trapping  of 
both  Pewmyscus  and  Clethrionomys  it  is  not  an  uncommon  experience  to 
find  either  of  these  mice  dead  in  a  trap  with  a  bloody  stump  of  one  hind 
leg,  while  another  trap,  2-5  feet  away,  is  covered  with  fresh  blood  and  fur. 
The  conclusion  as  to  what  happened  is  clear.  The  mouse  happened  to  get 
caught  in  one  trap  by  one  leg,  it  chewed  or  pulled  itseh  loose,  then  went 
fairly  directly  to  another  trap,  bit  at  the  bait  on  the  treadle  and  was  thus 
killed.  If  these  mice  have  a  strange-object  reaction  it  must  be  of  a  suffi- 
ciently low  order  of  magnitude  that  even  the  recent  loss  of  a  leg  in  one  trap 
fails  to  increase  it  to  the  point  of  avoiding  the  next  trap  encountered. 

In  the  third  area.  Alberta,  from  which  adequate  data  are  available  for 
these  two  genera,  both  are  relatively  abundant  but  Pewmyscus  exceeds 
Clethrionomys  (Table  VII).  Dming  the  fall  season,  nearly  twice  as  many 
of  each  genus  are  trapped  as  during  the  spring.  During  the  period  of  low 
spring  densities,  these  genera  exhibit  nearly  identical  rates  of  decline 
(Fig.  15 A)  accompanying  removal  trapping.  As  with  the  Maine  data,  such 
trends  may  be  interpreted  as  indicating  that  at  such  densities  neither 
species  is  capable  of  markedly  inhibiting  the  home  range  of  the  other. 
However,  by  fall  many  Clethrionomys  have  contracted  their  home  ranges 
as  a  response  to  their  exposure  not  only  to  more  of  their  own  kind  but  also 
to  more  Peromyscus.  Home  ranges  of  Peromyscus  remained  unaltered,  as 
indicated  by  the  similarity  of  rate  of  decline  during  both  spring  and  fall 
(Fig.  15).  However,  the  daily  catch  for  Clethrionomys  remained  nearly 
identical  through  three  successive  days  of  removal  trapping.  Sufficient  en- 
largement of  home  ranges  by  surviving  Clethrionomys  must  have  taken 
place  each  day  to  lead  to  an  equivalent  frequency  of  traps  being  encountered 
on  the  following  day  bj^  red-backed  mice,  despite  their  fewer  numbers 


1.   The  Social  Use  of  Space  55 

than  on  the  preceding  day.  At  this  Alberta  site,  the  inhibition  of  Clethri- 
onomys  home  ranges  by  Peromyscus  must  have  been  less  than  the  inhibition 
of  home  ranges  of  Peromyscus  by  Clethrionomys  in  the  Adirondacks. 

Despite  the  paucity  of  areas  from  which  extensive  comparable  data  are 
available,  it  looks  as  though  Clethrionomys  has  a  slight  advantage  over 
Peromyscus  in  gaining  psychological  ascendency.  At  this  point  one  may 
suspect  that  the  characteristics  of  some  environments  will  markedly  favor 
the  reproduction  and  survival  of  one  of  these  genera.  Whichever  genus 
this  happens  to  be  will  then  become  psychologically  dominant  to  the  other, 
as  evinced  by  the  contraction  of  home  range  of  the  less  numerous  genus. 
Consideration  of  other  aspects  of  the  relationship  between  these  two 
genera  requires  familiarity  with  the  concept  of  the  constellation  dealt 
with  in  the  following  sections. 

B.  Removal  Captures  of  Socially  Dominant  Species 

In  several  studies  already  presented  (Figs.  9-13)  we  have  seen  that  one 
species  tends  to  be  caught  in  large  numbers  during  the  first  few  days,  and 
that  the  time  of  maximum  input  for  the  remaining  species  comes  during  a 
successively  later  period.  Very  frequently  a  secondary  increase  in  daily 
catch  starts  near  the  1  oth  day  of  trapping  for  the  species  with  initially  the 
greatest  rate  of  capture.  Such  species  will  henceforth  be  designated  as  the 
socially  dominant  or  alpha  species  of  the  small  mammal  community.  For 
example,  see  Cleihrionoviys  in  Fig.  9  and  Perotnyscus  in  Fig.  10.  Where 
there  are  several  species  taken,  usually  only  one  is  characterized  by  this 
secondary  input.  Four  censuses  examined  included  one  such  species  and 
a  fifth  included  two  with  definite  secondary  inputs.^ 

Although  several  species  are  involved  in  this  phenomenon,  the  assump- 
tion is  here  made  that  they  all  so  behave  because  of  similar  properties 
leading  to  their  alpha  rank  in  the  community.  If  this  is  so,  we  are  justified 
in  pooling  the  data.  A  table  of  the  total  catch  per  day  of  trapping  was 

^  The  five  censuses  utilized  in  preparing  Fig.  24: 

1.  By  Dr.  J.  E.  Moore,  Sept.  1959,  Edmonton,  Alberta:  128  Peromyscus  maniculatus 
borealis,  65%  of  30-day  total  by  day  14-15. 

2.  By  Dr.  A.  I.  Roest,  Oct.-Nov.  1959,  San  Luis  Obispo,  California:  75  Dipodomys 
heermanni,  65%  of  30-day  total  b}^  day  15-16. 

3.  By  Dr.  William  L.  Webb,  Fall  1952,  Rich  Lake  Island,  Newcomb,  New  York: 
97  Clethrionomys  g.  gapperi,  65%  of  30-day  total  by  day  7. 

4.  By  Dr.  Earl  F.  Patric,  Fall  1953,  Arbutus  Area,  Newcomb,  New  York:  86 
Clethrionomys  g.  gapperi,  65%  of  30-day  total  by  day  12-13. 

5.  By  Dr.  Kyle  R.  Barbehenn,  Nov.-Dec.  1959,  Chadwick  Woods,  Montgomery 
County,  Maryland:  57  Peromyscus  leucopus,  65%  of  30-day  total  by  day  14; 
116  Blarina  brevicauda,  65%  of  30-day  total  by  day  20-21. 


56 


John  B.  Calhoun 


prepared.  These  data  are  shown  as  a  three-point  moving  average  in  Fig.  24. 
There  results  a  continuous  decHne  in  catch  until  about  the  12th  day. 
After  this,  the  daily  rate  of  capture  increases,  reaching  a  second  maxi- 
mum five  to  seven  days  later.  Following  this,  the  number  of  animals  taken 
continuously  declines,  but  it  is  not  until  about  the  27th  day  of  removal 
trapping  that  the  numbers  taken  per  day  reaches  the  low  level  character- 
izing the  12th  day. 


1          1 

MAINLY  aa/3 

1                     1                     1 
MAINLY    Y 

60 

1 

INDIVIDUALS 

INDIVIDUALS 

g   50 

q: 

UJ 

a. 

\ 

X 

o 

S  40 
o 

- 

1  • 

_ 

o 

\ 

• 

o  30 
IT 

tu 

- 

\» 

- 

1- 

§20 

lO 

- 

\   * 
^ 

•  • 
7                     •                *N,^ 

- 

10 

\^ 

•X. 

*v 

0 

1                     1 

1           1           1 

0  5  10  15  20  25  30 

DAY   OF  TRAPPING 

Fig.  24.  Removal  captures  of  559  small  mammals  who  are  representatives  of  species, 
socially  domiuant  in  their  community.  Alpha  and  beta  represent  the  intraspecific  domi- 
nant individuals  with  large  home  ranges.  The  gamma  individuals  represent  the  intra- 
specific subordinates  which  enlarge  their  home  ranges  following  removal  of  the  alphas 
and  betas. 


If  the  assumption  that  practically  all  the  resident  population  exposed  to 
the  traps  is  removed  diu-ing  30  days  of  trapping  is  correct,  then  the  accu- 
mulated catch  plotted  as  the  proportion  of  the  30-day  total  over  time  will 
reveal  both  the  proportion  of  the  total  comprising  the  initial  input  and  the 
time  at  which  the  secondary  input  begins  (Fig.  25).  This  shows  that  the 
intersection  of  the  two  rates  of  input  occurs  on  day  15  after  removal  of 
64%  of  the  resident  population. 


1.   The  Social  Use  of  Space 

C.  Constellation  Formation — An  Intraspecific  Phenomenon 


57 


After  the  removal  of  64%  of  the  resident  population  of  socially  dominant 
species,  whose  home  ranges  are  sufficiently  large  to  give  them  a  higher 
probability  of  encountering  traps,  there  arises  a  secondary  input.  These 
latter  individuals  must  have  enlarged  their  home  ranges  as  a  response  to 
the  absence  of  their  former  associates.  It  follows  that  certain  individuals 


_I 

i                     1 

'             '  .V"*'^ 

<    0.9 

1 — 

j§               — 

1- 

J* 

o 

yi 

> 

ym 

g    0.8 

- 

V» 

6 

^Z 

ro 

/ 

¥    0.7 

- 

/ 

1- 

J 

Ll. 

^ 

' 

o 

p^ 

Z  0.6 

—                                    ^y^ 

_ 

o 

jm^ 

1- 

y^ 

(£ 

0 

O 

/ 

S    0.5 

_                        /• 

- 

tr 

u 

Q. 

/ 

< 

i 

v)  0.4 

—              1 

— 

< 

L 

X 

1 

o 

<    0.3 

1 

_      L 

_ 

o 

1 

o 

lU 

So. 

1 

-A 

- 

13 

/ 

2 

A 

Z> 

^ 

o 

1 

o    0.1 

t— 



< 

n 

1             1 

I                        1 

15 
DAY 


20 


Fig.  25.  Removal  capture.s  of  socially  dominant  species.  Same  data  as  in  Fig.  24, 
but  here  shown  as  an  accumulated  total. 

have  the  ability  to  restrict  the  home  ranges  of  other  members  of  their  own 
species.  At  this  point  in  my  analysis  I  began  to  wonder  whether  there  were 
circumstances  relating  to  the  spatial  distribution  of  the  population  which 
might  lead  to  a  64:36  ratio  of  larger :  smaller  home  ranges. 

In  the  previous  discussion  we  have  seen  that  one  possible  spatial  equilib- 
rium is  that  in  which  home  range  centers  become  uniformly  distributed 
with  an  inter-home  range  center  interval  of  about  2.0  sigma.  It  was  further 
pointed  out  that  the  mechanics  of  moving  through  space  are  such  as  to 
favor  the  evolution  of  a  signal  emitted  by  one  individual  which  has  the 


58 


John  B.  Calhoun 


potentiality  of  eliciting  a  response  by  another  up  to  a  maximal  distance  of 
about  3  sigma  between  individuals.  We  may  then  ask,  "How  many  other 
individuals  in  such  a  uniformly  distributed  population  can  any  one  in- 
dividual know?"  Figure  26  shows  that  a  line  connecting  the  home  range 
center  of  any  one  individual's  six  nearest  neighbors  forms  a  hexagon.  With 
the  hypothesized  communication  system  operating,  this  one  individual 
can  remain  in  constant  communication  contact  with  all  its  six  nearest 
neighbors  except  under  the  condition  when  it  and  one  of  them  happen  to 


Fig.  26.  Spatial  distribution  of  home  range  centers  during  the  preconstellation  phase. 
Small  open  circles  represent  centers  for  individuals  destined  to  become  alpha  members. 
Dots  represent  home  range  centers  for  all  other  individuals.  Home  range  centers  are 
uniformlj'  distributed. 


move  in  opposite  directions  away  from  the  line  connecting  their  respective 
home  range  centers.  Such  actions  can  increase  the  distance  between  them 
to  greater  than  3  sigma. 

Lines  connecting  the  home  range  centers  of  this  individual's  next-nearest 
neighbors  also  form  a  hexagon.  Home  range  centers  for  all  these  twelve 
next-nearest  neighbors  lie  nearly  4.0  sigma  from  the  selected  individual. 
Therefore,  each  of  these  next-nearest  neighbors  can  be  in  contact  with  the 
selected  individual  when  they  approach  each  other,  such  that  the  distance 
intervening  between  them  becomes  less  than  3.0  sigma.  Any  more-distant 
neighbors,  those  living  G.O  or  more  sigma  from  the  selected  one,  will  have 


1.  The  Social  Use  of  Space  59 

such  a  low  frequency  of  communication  with  the  selected  individual  that 
mutual  effects  will  be  of  little  significance. 

Examination  of  the  spatial  distribution  of  home  range  centers  in  a  uni- 
formly distributed  population  reveals  that  one-twelfth  of  the  members 
each  have  six  nearest  neighbors  whom  they  can  influence  without  competi- 
tive influence  being  exerted  by  any  other  individuals  in  this  one-twelfth 
population.  Such  individuals  will  hereafter  be  designated  as  alpha  indi- 
viduals. Home  range  centers  for  these  alpha  individuals  are  designated  by 
small  open  circles  in  Fig.  26.  It  will  further  be  noted  that  two  such  alpha 
individuals,  who  live  closest  to  each  other,  share  next-nearest  neighbors. 

Antagonistic  relations,  uniform  utilization  of  the  environment,  and 
attainment  of  uniform  sign  and  signal  fields  approximating  in  intensity 
the  sign  and  signal  intensity  expressed  by  an  individual  at  its  own  home 
range  center,  and  eciualization  of  contact  rate  with  all  neighbors  throughout 
any  one  individual  range,  all  serve  as  forces  leading  to  a  uniform  distribu- 
tion of  home  range  centers  at  near  2.0  sigma  between  centers  (Section 

VII,  K) .  Yet,  we  may  anticipate  an  opposing  force  of  attraction  between 
members  in  close  communication  with  each  other.  This  will  lead  to  all 
nearest  neighbors  shifting  the  centers  of  their  home  ranges  slightly  toward 
their  single  alpha  associate.  Such  nearest  neighbors  will  hereafter  be 
designated  as  beta  individuals. 

Now  we  can  turn  our  attention  to  the  alpha's  next-nearest  neighbors. 
It  may  be  seen  from  Fig.  26  that  in  each  group  of  twelve  next-nearest 
neighbors,  six  are  eciuidistant  to  two  neighboring  alphas.  Therefore,  since 
the  probability  of  each  of  these  six  nearest  neighbors  is  0.5  of  being  at- 
tracted to  either  of  two  alpha  individuals  to  which  they  are  eciuidistant, 
each  alpha  on  the  average  will  have  attracted  to  it  three  of  these  next- 
nearest  neighbors.  The  remaining  six  next-nearest  neighbors  to  any  alpha 
are  ecjuidistant  to  three  alphas.  It  similarly  follows  that  on  the  average 
any  one  alpha  will  have  attracted  to  it  two  of  six  such  next-nearest  neigh- 
bors. Next-nearest  neighbors  w^hich  so  move  toward  an  alpha  associate 
will  hereafter  be  designated  as  gamma  individuals.  Thus,  each  alpha  will 
have  attracted  toward  it  six  beta  individuals  and  on  the  average  five 
gamma  individuals.  This  process  leads  to  a  clumping  of  the  population 
into  groups  ranging  in  size  from  7  to  19,  with  a  mean  of  12  (see  Section 

VIII.  D). 

The  central  alpha  indi\'idual  because  of  its  more  favored  position  in  the 
communication  network  may  be  assumed  to  be  dominant  to  both  its  beta 
and  gamma  associates.  Gamma  individuals,  because  of  their  peripheral 
location  in  the  developing  clump,  here  designated  a  constellation,  will  be 
subordinate  to  both  the  beta  and  alpha  associates.  Now  let  us  consider  the 


60  John  B.  Calhoun 

situation  where  home  ranges  contract  as  individuals  become  more 
subordinate. 

How  much  beta  and  gamma  members  may  be  expected  to  contract 
their  home  ranges  requires  consideration  of  phenomena  treated  in  Section 
XIV,  E,  titled  "Velocity  and  Home  Range."  There,  it  is  shown  that  in 
terms  of  o-„  units  of  distance,  the  home  range  sigmas  for  alpha,  beta,  and 
gamma  individuals  become,  respectively,  1.0,  0.9575,  and  0.6457.  Since 
an  optimum  uniform  utilization  of  the  environment  requires  a  2a  interval 
between  home  range  centers,  the  distance  between  alpha  and  beta  home 
range  centers  wall  become  1.9575o-a,  and  1.6032o-a  between  beta  and  gamma 
home  range  centers. 

The  periphery  of  each  constellation  (Fig.  27)  may  be  arbitrarily  desig- 
nated as  that  radius  from  the  center  of  the  alpha  member's  home  range 
extending  to  one  aa  beyond  the  center  of  each  gamma  individual's  home 
range.  This  radius  is  3.1478a-a.  And  since  the  home  range  centers  of  the 
alpha  members  of  adjoining  constellations  are  6.92o-a  apart,  it  is  obvious 
that  between  constellations  there  lies  what  may  be  called  an  interconstella- 
tion  matrix,  receiving  very  little  usage  from  the  dominant  species  forming 
constellations.  Furthermore,  this  interconstellation  matrix  must  markedly 
reduce  communication  between  members  of  adjoining  constellations.  How 
extensive  this  reduction  becomes  must  be  viewed  against  the  requirement 
of  a  2a  inter-HRC  interval  for  an  optimum  state.  Yet,  the  HRC  of  any 
beta  or  gamma  member  of  one  constellation  will  lie  on  the  average  4a  units 
of  distance  away  from  the  nearest  beta  or  gamma  members  of  an  adjoining 
constellation  with  reference  to  their  own  reduced  home  ranges. 

On  the  average,  such  constellations  will  consist  of  twelve  individuals,  1 
alpha,  6  betas,  and  5  gammas.  Of  these,  the  gammas  with  smallest  home 
ranges  form  0.417  of  the  population.  These  are  the  individuals  who,  after 
removal  of  their  dominant  alpha  and  beta  associates,  will  enlarge  their 
home  ranges.  Actually,  some  gammas  will  be  trapped  before  the  15th  day 
and  some  alphas  and  betas  will  be  taken  after  it.  Furthermore,  Fig.  25 
clearly  shows  that  a  few  more  gammas  would  have  been  taken  after  the 
30th  day  of  trapping.  Therefore,  the  observed  proportion  of  the  population 
consisting  of  gamma  individuals,  estimated  at  about  36%,  reasonably  well 
approximates  the  theoretical  expected  of  41.7%- 

To  date,  this  approximation  stands  as  the  only  direct  supporting  proof 
(see  Section  XI)  that  populations  of  dominant  species  of  small  mammal 
communities  do,  in  fact,  tend  to  form  such  constellations.  If  they  really 
do,  we  may  anticipate  that  there  has  been  evolution  relative  to  physiology 
such  that  its  optimum  state  is  most  compatible  with  interactions  among 
individuals  transpiring  in  groups  with  a  mean  size  of  twelve  adults.  In 


7.   The  Social  Use  of  Space 


61 


later  sections,  I  shall  present  data  which  support  the  hypothesis  that  a 
group  size  of  twelve  has  been  phylogenetically  retained  in  more  highly 
evolved  forms. 

For  the  present,  let  us  examine  fm'ther  implications  of  constellation 
formation  upon  the  structure  of  the  small  mammal  community.   Con- 


FiG.  27.  The  theoretical  constellation  phase  of  intraspecific  community  organization 
of  dominant  species.  A  3-sigma  radius  home  range  for  alpha  individuals  is  z-epresented 
by  the  large  circle.  Small  circles  represent  1-sigma  radius  portions  of  all  members  of  a 
constellation.  Contraction  of  home  range  by  beta  and  alpha  members  permits  the  more 
intensively  used  portion  of  the  home  ranges  of  all  individuals  to  fall  mostly  within  the 
3-sigma  radius  home  range  of  the  dominant  alpha  members.  Crosshatched  circles 
represent  the  1-sigma  portion  of  the  home  range  of  alpha  individuals. 


62  John  B.  Calhoun 

stellatioii  formation  presupposes  passing  through  a  stage  of  uniform  dis- 
tribution of  home  ranges.  In  the  interstices  between  the  home  ranges  of 
alpha  species  there  is  the  opportunity  for  very  subordinate  species  with 
highly  contracted  home  ranges  to  establish  themselves.  As  the  constellation 
forms,  certain  members  of  the  subordinate  species  will  be  retained  within 
the  interstices  of  the  constellation  while  others  will  lie  at  its  periphery. 
However,  as  constellations  form,  there  develops  an  interconstellation 
matrix  rarely  frequented  by  members  of  the  alpha  species.  We  may  then 
wonder  what  members  of  the  community  will  live  here. 

At  this  point,  the  Huntington  Forest  censuses  of  1952  and  1953  (Fig.  11) 
are  particularly  instructive.  The  red-backed  mouse,  Clethrionomys,  is  ob- 
viously the  alpha  species.  Just  as  obviously,  the  shrew  Sorex  is  the  most 
subordinate  species,  which  has  highly  contracted  home  ranges  lying  in  the 
interstices  between  those  of  Clethrionomys.  Had  all  these  census  been  run 
for  longer  than  30  days,  it  looks  as  though  the  2 : 1  ratio  of  the  alpha  species 
to  one  of  its  most  subordinate  associates  would  have  been  realized.  Censuses 
shown  from  other  areas  indicate  that  both  Peromyscus  and  Blarina  are 
dominant  to  Sorex  in  the  community.  And  yet,  in  these  Huntington 
Forest  censuses,  relatively  few  of  either  were  caught.  What  I  suspect  has 
happened  is  that  as  the  constellations  developed,  both  species  were  relegated 
to  the  interconstellation  matrix,  where  they  existed  in  somewhat  contracted 
home  ranges,  as  indicated  by  the  catch  per  day  increasing  from  day  1  to 
days  4-6.  In  fact,  for  both  these  species  it  looks  as  though  a  certain  portion 
of  their  members  had  even  more  markedly  contracted  home  ranges.  This 
is  indicated  by  the  secondary  increase  in  catch  per  day  starting  betw^een 
days  8  and  10. 


D.  Expected     Variability     in     the     Number     of    Individuals     Forming 
Constellations 

In  terms  of  the  formulation  of  Section  VIII,  C  the  six  nearest  neighbors 
to  any  alpha  individual  will  always  be  attracted  toward  it  as  the  members 
of  a  population  wuth  a  uniform  distribution  of  home  range  centers  begin 
to  form  diffuse  clumps,  termed  constellations.  The  twelve  next-nearest 
neighbors  to  any  alpha  individuals  may  be  divided  into  two  types,  a  and  h, 
according  to  their  probability  of  being  attracted  toward  any  given  alpha 
neighbor. 

Let:  a  represent  the  six  individuals  who  have  a  probability,  pa  =   2,  of 
being  attracted  to  any  given  alpha. 
h  represent  the  six  individuals  who  have  a  probability,  'Pb  =  k,  of 
being  attracted  to  any  given  alpha. 


1.   The  Social  Use  of  Space 


63 


The  probability,  Pa{i),  that  i  number  of  the  six  a  individuals  will  be  at- 
tracted to  the  given  alpha  will  be : 


Pa(i) 


(A  /I 


(28) 


Similarly  the  probability,  pb(i),  that  i  number  of  the  six  b  individuals  will 
be  attracted  to  the  given  alpha  wdll  be: 


Pb(i)  = 


il  \3/   \3 


'    /9\^— * 


(29) 


Therefore,  the  probability,  p{m),  where  m  =  0  to  12,  of  m  members  of 
the  12  next-nearest  neighbors  joining  any  given  alpha  and  its  six  nearest 
neighbors  to  form  a  constellation  will  be: 


p{m)   =   ^  Paii)  '  Pbim  -  i) 


(30) 


For  example,  if  m  =  4  this  becomes: 

P(4)     =    PaiO)     '    Pbi^)     +    Pail)     '    Pt(:i)     +    Pa{'2)     '    p,{2)     +    p„(3) 

•   P5(l)    +p„(4)    .   PbiQ)        (31) 

p(S))  •  •  •  p{12)  were  calculated  and  are  shown  in  Table  VIII  as  p{7  +  0) 

Table  VIII 
Expected  ^'ARIABILITy  ix  the  Number  of  Individuals  Formixg  a  Constellation 


Xumbor  of  animals  in  group 


Prol:)abilit3'  of  group  developing 


9 
10 
11 
12 
13 
14 
15 
16 
17 
18 
19 


0.00137200 
0.01234568 
0.05041152 
0.12345679 
0.20190329 
0.23225308 
0.19266546 
0.11612654 
0.05047582 
0.01543210 
0.00315072 
0.00038580 
0.00002143 


Z  1.00000023 


64  John  B.  Calhoun 

•  •  •  p  (7  +  12)  since  the  constellation  becomes  composed  of  the  addition 
of  these  next-nearest  neighbors,  or  gamma  individuals  to  the  basic  core  of 
the  one  alpha  and  its  six  beta  nearest  neighbors. 


E.  Social  Rank  and  Intraspecific  Associations 

In  the  14-day  removal  study  shown  in  Fig.  13B,  trapping  results  indi- 
cated large  home  ranges  for  Peromyscus,  moderate-sized  ones  for  Blarina, 
and  highly  contracted  home  ranges  for  Pitymys.  Estimated  home  range 
sigmas,  o-,  of  50,  25,  and  12.5  feet,  respectively,  probably  closely  enough 
approximate  the  real  values  to  permit  their  use  in  a  study  of  these  data. 
If  we  knew  the  actual  home  range  centers  of  all  animals  trapped,  a  1-sigma 
radius  circle  plotted  about  each  on  a  map  of  the  study  area  should  pro\'ide 
more  insight  into  spatial  relationships. 

Since  such  centers  were  not  known,  they  were  approximated  by  making 
the  following  assumptions: 

1.  Peromyscus  maximize  inter-home-range-center   distance   from  other 
Peromyscus. 

2.  Blarina  similarly  maximize  distance,  not  only  from  others  of  theu' 
own  kind,  but  also  from  Peromyscus. 

3.  Pitymys  maximize  distance  from  both  Peromyscus  and  Blarina,  as 
well  as  from  others  of  their  own  kind. 

4.  The  later  an  animal  was  trapped  the  farther  its  home  range  centers 
were  located  from  the  trapline. 

For  Peromyscus  some  of  the  HRC's  were  shifted  to  one  side  of  the  line 
of  capture,  and  the  remaining  to  the  other  side,  until  ever}''  adjacent  3 
HRC's  approximated  equilateral  triangles.  A  similar  procedure  was  ap- 
plied to  Blarina  captures  except  that  where  possible  their  HRC's  also 
were  placed  in  the  center  of  triangles  formed  by  the  HRC's  for  Peromyscus. 
Then  the  HRC's  for  Pitymys  w^ere,  insofar  as  possible,  placed  in  the  centers 
of  triangles  connecting  the  HRC's  of  the  other  two  species. 

Utilizing  these  assumptions,  centers  of  home  ranges  for  Peromyscus 
were  plotted  first,  Blarina  second,  and  Pitymys  last.  Originally  these  were 
all  plotted  on  a  single  figure,  but  for  the  sake  of  greater  clarity  in  examining 
intraspecific  relations  the  home  ranges,  in  terms  of  1-sigma  circles,  are 
shown  separately  in  Figs.  28-30. 

Moisture  conditions  in  the  habitat  varied  markedly.  Steep  xerophytic 
slopes  of  oak  and  pine  covered  the  three  sectors  encompassed  between  the 
E  and  SW  radii.  The  sector  between  the  N  and  NW  radii  was  quite  steep 
and  dry.  A  damp  drainage  area  lay  roughly  along  the  W  and  NE  radii. 


L   The  Social  Use  of  Space 


65 


Fig.  28.   Schematic  home  ranges  for  Peromyscus.  The  octagons  represent  traplines 
along  which  removal  trapping  was  conducted. 


<r«25   FEET  N 


Fig.  29.   Schematic  home  ranges  for  Blarina. 


66 


John  B.  Calhoun 


Away  from  the  nari-ow  drainage  area  the  other  four  sectors  were  moderately 
mesophytic.  Deep  leaf  mold  covered  the  entire  forest  floor.  No  attempt  was 
made  to  obtain  a  detailed  cover  map.  Total  timbering  a  year  later  for  a 
housing  development  revealed  that  many  of  the  trees  in  the  forest  exceeded 
150  years  in  age. 

It  may  be  noted  that  both  Peromyscus  and  Blarina  tended  to  avoid  the 
more  xerophytic  areas,  whereas  Pitymys  was  more  abundant  in  these 
drier  areas.  However,  it  is  interesting  to  note  that  no  Blarina  occurred  in 
the  N  to  NW  sector  and  yet  quite  a  number  were  taken  in  the  even  more 


a  =  l2.5  FEET  N 


O      jO        o 

s 
Fig.  30.   Schematic  home  ranges  for  Pitymys. 


xerophytic  E  to  SW  sectors.  The  marked  clumping  of  Pitymys  in  the  N  to 
NW  sector  suggests  that  a  tendency  toward  colony  formation  in  this  very 
subordinate  species  may  actually  serve  to  exclude  from  that  region  the 
more  dominant  Blarina. 

However,  despite  such  a  local  tendency  for  Pitymys  to  cluster,  no  over- 
lapping of  the  1-sigma  radius  home  ranges  occurred  anywhere  in  the  study 
plot.  In  fact,  the  mean  interval  between  home  range  centers  of  nearest 
neighbors  is  of  the  order  of  4  sigma.  This  must  mean  that  the  probability  of 
one  Pitymys  associating  with  a  neighbor  is  very  low.  With  their  larger 
home  ranges,  there  is  more  opportunity  for  Blarina  to  associate  with  others 


1.   The  Social  Use  of  Space  67 

of  their  own  kind.  Yet  even  with  them  their  movements  are  sufficiently 
inhibited  as  to  reduce  contacts  far  below  that  otherwise  possible.  How- 
ever, with  Peromyscus  so  much  overlapping  of  home  ranges  existed  that 
most  individuals  must  have  had  frequent  associations  with  others  of  their 
own  kind.  Reduction  of  communication  with  others  of  their  own  kind  ac- 
companies interspecific  social  subordination.  The  lower  the  rank  of  a 
species,  in  terms  of  the  degree  other  species  in  the  community  cause  it  to 
restrict  its  home  range,  the  more  its  intraspecific  systems  of  communication 
will  be  reduced.  Even,  as  the  analyses  shown  in  Figs.  28-30  suggest,  if 
there  develops  some  compensatory  clumping  of  home  ranges  by  members 
of  subordinate  species,  there  must  still  be  a  greater  degree  of  isolation 
between  such  clumps  than  between  any  comparable  number  of  groups  of 
the  dominant  species. 


F.  The  Instability  of  Social  Relations 

Results  from  the  extensive  censusing  of  small  mammals  conducted  by 
Drs.  Patric  and  Webb  and  their  associates  at  the  Huntington  Forest  form 
a  major  key  in  unraveling  the  process  involved  in  community  structure. 
Fortunately,  their  records  (Patric,  1958)  include  some  of  the  years  before 
1952  (see  Table  VI  for  1952-1956) . 

An  effort  of  9650  trap-nights  during  1940  and  1941,  in  which  traps  were 
set  for  five  consecutive  nights,  caught  173  Clethrionomys  and  1280  Peromys- 
cus, or  7.4  Peromyscus  for  each  Clethrionomys;  in  contrast,  during  the 
years  of  1952-1956,  4.1  Clethrionomys  were  captured  for  each  Peromyscus 
taken  (Table  VI).  However,  during  these  two  years  the  trends  of  capture 
over  time  were  so  nearly  identical  that  only  the  greater  numbers  of  Pero- 
myscus can  argue  for  its  having  been  more  dominant.  Actually,  both 
showed  increases  in  catch  per  day  as  associates  were  removed.  The  day  1 
to  day  5  captures  were:  for  Peromyscus,  117,  225,  313,  317,  308;  for  Clethri- 
onomys, 17,  19,  61,  46,  30.  Apparently  many  members  of  both  species  were 
characterized  by  reduced  home  ranges,  which  they  expanded  as  associates 
were  removed.  There  being  so  few  Clethrionomys  present,  they  could  not 
represent  the  species  producing  the  inhibition  of  home  ranges.  Thus  at  this 
time  we  must  suspect  that  actions  by  members  of  the  genus  Peromyscus 
not  only  caused  many  of  its  own  kind  to  contract  their  home  ranges,  but 
also  caused  similar  contraction  by  Clethrionomys.  In  the  light  of  the  ap- 
parent reversibility  of  social  roles  of  these  two  genera,  previously  discussed 
when  comparing  different  localities,  these  comparisons  between  different 
eras  within  the  same  habitat  suggest  that  Peromyscus  and  Clethrionomys 
in  this  habitat  are  really  codominants,  but  that  one  will  nearly  exclude  the 


68  John  B.  Calhoun 

other.  At  such  times  whichever  one  became  more  numerous  than  the  other 
would  cause  members  of  the  less  numerous  species  to  contract  their  home 
ranges  and  live  within  the  interconstellation  matrix  of  the  more  abundant 
species. 

A  small  amount  of  trapping  during  1946-1948  revealed  that  Peromyscus 
still  held  a  3:1  relative  abundance  over  Cleihrionomijs.  The  intensive  trap- 
ping was  resumed  during  1951,  an  apparently  critical  year  in  the  social 
balance  of  the  population.  Forty-one  NACSM  traplines,  each  run  for  three 
consecutive  days,  for  a  total  of  7380  trap-nights  of  effort  (NACSM  Re- 
lease No.  5)  provided  total  catches  for  the  three  consecutive  days  as 
follows:  Clethrionomys:  143,  114,  77;  Peromyscus:  167,  132,  108;  Blarina: 
52,  58,  50;  Sorex:  2,  0,  2.  Although  Peromyscus  was  slightly  more  numerous 
than  Clethrionomys,  9.93  versus  8.15  per  trapline  per  three  days,  its  slower 
rate  of  decline  in  captures  from  days  1  through  3  suggests  that  it  was 
slightly  subordinate  and  that  some  of  its  members  were  enlarging  home 
ranges  as  their  associates  were  being  trapped  off.  However,  the  nearly 
constant  total  catch  per  day  for  Blarina  clearly  indicated  its  subordinate 
status  to  the  other  two  genera. 

The  24-day  continuous  removal  study  of  1951  (Table  V,  Fig.  12)  pro- 
vided further  insight  into  this  realignment  of  social  relations.  Despite 
nearly  equal  numbers  taken  for  the  three  most  abundant  species,  Blarina 
is  clearly  subordinate  to  both  Peromyscus  and  Clethrionomys.  Its  more 
marked  contraction  of  home  ranges  is  revealed  by  the  continuously  in- 
creasing daily  catch  over  the  first  few  days  of  trapping  (Fig.  12) .  Compari- 
son of  the  proportion  taken  the  first  5  days  with  that  during  the  last  5 
days  provides  an  index  of  expansion  of  home  range.  The  relatively  more 
that  are  taken  during  the  first  5  days,  the  less  has  been  the  expansion  of 
home  range,  and  thus  the  more  dominant  the  species.  On  this  basis,  the 
four  genera  are  listed  in  order  of  decreasing  rank  in  Table  V. 

One  of  the  most  remarkable  aspects  of  this  set  of  data  is  the  nearly  con- 
stant daily  catch  for  each  of  the  three  more  abundant  genera  between  the 
4th  and  17th  day  of  trapping.  This  means  that  within  each  genus,  survivors 
increase  their  home  range  each  day  sufficiently  to  result  in  as  many  en- 
countering traps  as  on  the  previous  day  despite  their  fewer  numbers. 
Furthermore,  an  individual  member  of  each  of  these  genera  must  be  re- 
ceptive to  the  inhibitory  signals  from  the  other  two  genera  as  well  as  of 
others  of  its  own  kind.  Otherwise,  the  capture  curves  would  have  resembled 
that  for  clearly  alpha  species  (Fig.  9). 

This  set  of  data  also  suggests  that  in  the  presence  of  several  more  domi- 
nant species  actively  contending  for  rank  status,  the  very  subordinate 
Sorex  not  only  is  markedly  reduced  in  numbers,  but  also  is  much  slower  in 
expanding  its  home  ranges.  Peak  captures,  and  thus  maximum  expansion 


1.  The  Social  Use  of  Space  69 

of  home  range,  did  not  take  place  until  day  21,  which  was  just  after  the 
final  maximum  expansion  of  home  range  for  the  other  three  species. 

By  the  following  year  (Table  lb,  NACSIM  Release  No.  6)  this  uncertain 
social  state  had  completely  clarified.  Three-day  totals  for  34  NACSM 
lines  were  Clethrionomys  (753),  Peromyscus  (38),  Sorex  (17),  and  Blarina 
(1).  Other  details  of  the  resultant  social  organization  have*already  been 
treated  in  Section  VIII,  A. 

The  studies  (e.g.,  Figs.  10  and  13B)  conducted  during  the  past  few 
years  in  Montgomery  County,  Maryland,  by  Dr.  Barbehenn  and  me, 
further  substantiate  the  indeterminancy  of  the  small-mammal  community 
as  a  dynamic  system.  We  have  mostly  sampled  woodlands  of  50  to  1000 
acres.  Peromyscus,  Blarina,  Sorex,  and  Pitymys  are  the  more  abundant 
species.  In  every  case  Peromyscus  is  the  dominant  species.  For  it,  the  largest 
catches  per  day  occur  during  the  first  few  days,  and  usually  from  day  1 
there  is  a  continuously  declining  catch  per  day  with  the  exception  of  the 
slight  secondary  increase  resulting  from  expansion  of  home  ranges  by 
gamma  individuals  (Fig.  10).  Blarina  is  also  nearly  universally  present. 
The  date  by  which  509f  of  the  total  is  trapped  consistently  arrives  several 
days  later  for  Blarina  than  for  Peromyscus.  In  actuality,  peak  numbers 
taken  per  day  usually  occur  several  days  after  initiation  of  trapping.  Thus, 
in  these  communities  the  home  ranges  of  most  Blarina  are  socially  con- 
tracted. When  either  Sorex  or  Pitymys  is  present,  they  are  definitely  sub- 
ordinate to  both  Peromyscus  and  Blarina  in  terms  of  the  degree  to  which 
their  home  ranges  are  contracted.  Their  peak  captures  per  day  never  occur 
until  after  most  of  the  two  dominants  have  been  removed.  Relative  num- 
bers are  quite  another  matter.  It  seems  to  be  purely  a  matter  of  chance 
whether  either  Pitymys  or  Sorex  is  present  in  any  particular  woodlot. 
Either,  both,  or  neither  may  be  present.  Their  absence  appears  not  to  be 
due  to  absence  of  requirements  for  food  and  shelter,  but  merely  due  to 
failure  to  reproduce  under  circumstances  of  spatial  isolation,  when  the 
processes  of  social  adjustment  within  a  particular  woodlot  happens  to 
markedly  reduce  the  numbers  of  some  one  species.  These  woodlots  in  subur- 
ban to  semirural  Montgomery  County,  adjacent  to  the  district  of  Columbia, 
exist  as  ecological  islands  which  must  be  characterized  by  a  rather  low 
probability  of  receiving  colonizers  of  these  subordinate  species.  Further- 
more, any  one  of  the  three  subordinate  species  can  become  the  most 
abundant  species  in  the  community.  In  each  case  the  most  abundant  species 
has  small  home  ranges  whose  centers  lie  within  the  interstices  of  the  larger 
home  ranges  of  the  dominant  Peromyscus  (as  shown  in  Fig.  IG).  Blarina 
was  such  a  species  in  the  study  shown  in  Fig.  10.  Had  trapping  been  con- 
tinued longer  in  the  study  shown  in  Fig.  13B,  Pitymys  would  undoubtedly 
have  had  a  nearly  2:1  ratio  of  abundance  to  the  dominant  Peromyscus. 


70  John  B.  Calhoun 

In  other  unpublished  studies  by  Dr.  Barbehenn,    Sorex  catches  nearly 
doubled  those  of  Peromyscus. 


IX.  A  Theoretical  Conceptualization  of  the  Evolution  of  a  Social  Hierarchy 
among  Species  in  the  Utilization  of  Space 

The  concept  of  social  inhibition  of  home  range  represents  an  inference 
derived  from  the  observed  differential  probability  of  capture  by  snap 
traps.  Reduction  of  extent  of  home  range  must  result  from  an  increase  in 
the  probability  of  terminating  trips  away  from  home.  We  have  seen  that 
an  increase  in  the  structuring  of  the  environment,  with  stimuli  which 
elicit  responses,  does  lead  to  an  increase  in  the  probability  of  terminating 
trips.  This  structuring  of  the  environment  represents  an  increase  in  the 
amount  of  stimuli  impinging  on  the  organism.  For  this  reason  I  believe 
that  one  is  justified  in  making  the  following  tentative  generalization:  Any 
increase  in  the  frequency  or  intensity  of  stimuli  to  which  the  animal  has 
responded  in  the  past  will  lead  to  an  increase  in  the  probability  of  termi- 
nating trips. 

It  follows  that  genera  such  as  Sorex  or  Pitymys  frequently  are  char- 
acterized by  such  small  home  ranges  that  we  may  conclude  that  they  ex- 
perience a  high  frequency  or  intensity  of  relevant  stimuli.  At  the  same  time 
their  associates,  Peromyscus  or  Clethrionomys,  hsixe  large  home  ranges. 
This  being  so,  we  may  conclude  that  the  latter  are  not  unduly  exposed  to 
an  excess  of  relevant  stimuli.  And  yet  we  may  suspect  that  any  stimulus 
which  Sorex  or  Pitymys  can  detect  can  also  be  detected  by  Peromyscus  or 
Clethyrionomys.  Thus,  their  differential  response  to  stimuli  must  involve 
some  internal  mechanism  through  which  stimuli  produce  an  effect  indicating 
relevancy  or  irrelevancy. 

So  far  it  has  appeared  that  auditory  stimuli  of  the  class  represented  by 
vocalizations  of  members  of  the  small  mammal  community  might  represent 
the  class  of  stimuli  producing  alterations  of  home  range.  If  this  inference 
proves  correct,  it  means  that  some  species  respond  to  a  broad  spectrum  of 
different  auditory  stimuli  whereas  others  "ignore"  all  except  those  emitted 
by  their  own  species. 

Broadbent  (1958)  elaborates  a  theory  of  perception  and  communication 
which  may  serve  in  conceptualizing  how  vocal  communication  functions  in 
leading  to  an  organization  of  the  small  mammal  community.  Briefly  this 
theory  is  as  follows: 

The  central  nervous  system  may  be  conceptualized  as  a  signal  flow 
system  possessing  the  following  connections  and  characteristics.  Stimuli 
impinging  upon  the  sense  organ  generate  signals  which  pass  into  a  tempo- 


1 


1.  The  Social  Use  of  Space  71 

rary  store.  Storage  here  is  in  terms  of  seconds  only.  From  this  short-term 
store,  signals  must  pass  through  a  hmited  capacity  channel  before  they 
can  serve  to  initiate  immediate  responses  or  reach  a  long-term  store  where 
the  signal  can  be  preserved  to  affect  later  action.  Signals  passing  through 
the  limited  capacity  channel  from  the  temporary  store  may  be  passed 
back  through  another  circuit  and  reenter  the  temporary  store.  Likewise, 
responses  made  to  a  stimulus  or  a  sequence  of  stimuli,  in  turn,  serve  as 
stimuli  which  generate  signals  entering  the  temporary  store.  Furthermore, 
several  signals  may  arrive  simultaneously  at  the  temporary  store  through 
separate  sensory  channels.  Only  a  portion  of  these  signals  in  the  tempo- 
rary store  can  get  through  the  limited  capacity  channel. 

There  has  evolved  a  neural  mechanism  which  Broadbent  calls  a  "filter," 
intervening  between  the  temporary  store  and  the  limited  capacity  channel. 
This  filter  "selects"  which  signals  may  get  through  the  limited  capacity 
channel  and  thus  be  available  for  (a)  recirculation  into  the  temporary 
store,  (b)  inducing  immediate  response,  or  (c)  entering  the  long-term 
store. 

The  following  conditions  affect  the  probability  of  signals  passing  through 
the  filter: 

1.  The  signal  is  of  the  same  class  as  that  of  the  prior  signal.  That  is, 
the  related  stimulus  has  similar  characteristics  in  terms  of  frequency,  in- 
tensity, pattern,  or  location  of  origin.  In  other  words,  the  filter  tends  to 
pass  in  sequence  several  signals  from  stimuli  with  related  characteristics. 

2.  However,  the  longer  a  given  category  of  signals  has  been  passing  the 
filter,  the  more  likely  the  filter  will  switch  to  signals  arriving  from  a  differ- 
ent sensory  channel. 

3.  Signals  generated  by  intense  or  infreciuent  (novel)  stimuli  e.xhibit  a 
high  probability  of  passing  through  the  filter. 

4.  Given  any  three  signals  in  the  temporary  store  and  one  is  passed 
through  the  filter,  the  one  of  the  remaining  two  most  likely  to  follow  it  is 
the  one  which  followed  it  most  frequently  on  prior  occasions. 

A  special  case  will  particularly  concern  us.  An  animal  may  exhibit  both 
bodily  response  and  vocalization  to  a  given  external  stimulus.  Each  of 
these  responses  also  becomes  a  stimulus  with  a  high  probability  of  associa- 
tion, each  with  the  other,  and  each  with  the  external  stimulus.  As  the  ex- 
ternal stimulus  becomes  weaker,  only  the  bodily  response  is  preserved. 
Presumably  the  reason  for  this  is  that  the  bodily  response  represents 
a  more  intense  stimulus  and  for  this  reason  develops  a  higher  conditional 
probability  of  association  with  the  external  stimulus.  That  is,  the  signal 
from  the  bodily  response  stimulus  is  more  likely  to  pass  through  the  filter 
immediately  after  the  signal  from  the  external  stimulus. 


72  John  B.  Calhoun 

Although  I  have  treated  Broadbent's  theory  only  sketchily  here,  its 
importance  in  the  context  of  the  present  discussion  is  his  documentation 
of  the  necessity  for  postulating  the  existence  of  some  neural  mechanism 
having  the  properties  he  attributes  to  the  "filter."  His  thesis  evolved 
primarily  from  studies  with  human  subjects,  and  to  a  much  lesser  extent 
from  studies  of  rats  and  dogs.  He  accepts  the  existence  of  a  filter,  and  even 
that  there  are  intraindividual  differences  in  the  effectiveness  of  its  matura- 
tion. His  concern  with  the  evolution  of  the  filter  extends  only  to  his  belief 
that  animals  with  a  smaller  cortex  probably  also  have  a  less  well  developed 
filter.  He  does  not  discuss  how  a  less  well  developed  filter  would  differ 
from  a  more  highly  evolved  one.  We  might  anticipate  the  four  attributes 
of  filters  listed  above  to  be  less  well  developed.  That  is,  signals  sequentially 
passing  the  filter  represent  a  class  of  stimuli  having  a  wider  range  of  varia- 
bility; shifts  from  one  sensory  channel  to  another  occur  with  greater  fre- 
ciuency;  a  stimulus  need  be  only  slightly  more  intense  or  novel  to  generate 
signals  capable  of  having  priority  over  other  signals  in  passing  through  the 
filter,  and  two  stimuli  must  be  associated  in  time  much  more  frequently 
for  their  signals  reaching  the  temporary  store  to  have  a  higher  conditional 
probability  of  passing  in  sequence  through  the  filter. 

Let  us  turn  to  a  consideration  of  how  the  social  use  of  space  may  have 
encouraged  the  evolution  of  animals  with  more  efficient  neural  filters.  At 
the  dawn  of  mammalian  evolution,  we  can  visualize  a  type  having  close 
equivalence  both  morphologically  and  physiologically  to  contemporary 
shrews  of  the  genus  Sorex,  with  the  exception  that  they  had  developed 
essentially  no  neural  filter  mechanism  for  screening  signals  passing  from 
the  temporary  store  to  the  limited  capacity  channel.  All  stimuli  arriving 
separately  would  get  through  the  limited  capacity  channel  and  it  was 
purely  a  matter  of  chance  which  of  two  simultaneously  arriving  stimuli 
might  find  passage.  Under  these  circumstances,  no  discrimination  may  be 
made  between  aversive  stimuli  emitted  by  neighbors  at  a  distance.  This 
would  lead  to  a  uniform  distribution  of  home  range  centers  at  approximately 
2.0  sigma  distance  between  centers. 

Paleontological  evidence  suggests  that  these  early  diminutive  mammals 
preyed  upon  insects  and  other  small  invertebrates.  Furthermore,  since 
among  present  day  shrews  Blarina  is  dominant  to  Sorex  in  the  hierarchy 
of  use  of  space,  it  follows  that  evolution  permitting  such  differential  con- 
trol of  space  must  have  proceeded  prior  to  further  marked  alteration  with 
reference  to  feeding  habits.  Therefore,  it  appears  that  early  in  the  history 
of  shrew-like  mammals  there  was  sufficient  evolution  to  permit  one  species 
to  inhibit  the  extent  of  home  range  of  another.  For  simplicity's  sake  let 
us  designate  the  earlier  form  as  A,  and  the  later  derived  one  as  B.  Two 
characteristics  gradually  become  fixed  in  B.  It  evolved  an  altered  vocaliza- 


1.  The  Social  Use  of  Space  73 

tion.  This  ^'Ocalization  preserved  much  of  the  characteristics  of  A,  but 
entailed  an  addition  of  components.  Following  Broadbent's  analysis  of 
stimulus  characteristics  we  may  suspect  that  an  attribute  of  some  portion 
of  the  added  vocal  components  included  an  increase  in  intensity.  Likewise, 
B's  filter  developed  alterations  which  enabled  B  to  filter  out  selectively 
those  vocalizations  not  including  the  new  attributes  developed  by  B.  Thus, 
members  of  the  new  species  B  could  develop  conditioned  associations  with 
the  vocal  signals  emitted  by  its  own  kind  while  ignoring  those  emitted  by 
species  A.  At  maximum  stability  of  such  a  two-species  community  there 
would  exist  a  2:1  ratio  of  A:B  in  a  similar  fashion  exemplified  by  the 
Blarina: Peromyscus  community  previously  described  for  Dr.  Barbehenn's 
Chadwick  Woods  study  (Section  VI,  B)  and  Dr.  Webb's  Rich  Lake 
Island  study  (Section  VI,  A)  for  Sorex: Clethrionomys. 

The  interesting  aspect  of  this  2 : 1  ratio  of  species  A:B  is  that  it  enabled 
three  animals  to  live  where  only  one  lived  before.  In  other  words,  the  e\'olu- 
tion  of  dominant  species  B  not  only  enabled  as  many  of  species  B  to  live 
in  the  habitat  as  was  formerly  the  case  with  reference  to  the  time  when 
species  A  only  existed  there,  but  it  also  enabled  twice  as  many  of  the  more 
primitive  species  A  to  live  in  the  habitat  as  had  been  the  case  when  B  was 
absent.  For  such  a  pattern  of  evolution  to  have  transpired,  it  means  that 
intraspecific  dispersal  of  home  range  centers  in  a  one-species  community 
as  a  consequence  of  the  repulsive  character  of  vocal  stimuli  must  have  been 
sufficient  to  ensure  an  average  utilization  of  resources  far  below  maximum 
carrying  capacity.  To  clarify  further  what  is  intimated  above:  Members 
of  A,  as  a  result  of  antagonistic  interactions  with  others  of  its  own  kind, 
develop  conditioned  avoidance  responses  to  intraspecific  vocalizations. 
Furthermore,  the  greater  the  frequency  of  these  vocalizations,  as  repre- 
sented by  increases  in  density  of  the  species,  the  greater  is  the  probability 
of  outward  excursions  from  home  being  terminated,  thus  the  smaller  home 
range.  Lacking  a  sufficiently  effective  neural  filter,  .-1  responds  to  B's 
vocalizations  as  if  they  were  their  own.  If  B  emits  signals  with  the  same 
frequency  as  does  A,  it  follows  that  in  a  stable  two-species  system  A  will 
be  responding  to  three  times  the  signal  load  as  B.  For  this  reason,  ^'s 
home  range  becomes  markedly  reduced  in  contrast  to  its  area  when  B 
was  absent. 

The  next  step  in  the  evolution  of  the  social  hierarchy  of  space  utilization, 
resulting  in  species  C,  entailed  similar  alterations  to  vocalizations  and  to 
enhancement  of  the  filter  in  restricting  the  spectrum  of  stimuli  which 
would  likely  be  associated  with  intraspecific  interactions.  Judging  by  the 
fact  that  the  omnivorous  mouse  Peromyscus  is  dominant  to  both  Sorex 
and  Blarina,  one  may  conclude  that  evolution  of  altered  food  preference 
facilitated  further  evolution  of  interspecific  social  domination  of  space. 


74  John  B.  Calhoun 

In  fact,  it  was  this  difference  in  food  preference  which  led  me  to  conchide 
that  the  interspecific  aspects  of  dominance  in  the  utihzation  of  space  most 
hkely  fails  to  involve  direct  physical  interaction  between  members  of 
different  species.  Objects  desired  by  species  holding  opposite  ranks  in  the 
hierarchy  gradually  became  more  and  more  different.  At  this  level  of  evolu- 
tion species  A  responds  not  only  to  vocalizations  of  its  own  kind  but  also 
responds  to  vocalizations  of  both  B  and  C  as  if  they  were  by  members  of 
species  A.  Species  B  can  ignore  signals  from  A  but  treats  both  its  own 
signals  and  those  from  C  as  B  signals,  while  C  "filters  out"  A'&  and  5's 
signals  and  responds  only  to  those  of  its  own  species.  In  other  words,  C 
functions  as  if  it  alone  were  in  the  environment. 

The  fourth  step  in\'olving  evolution  of  species  of  type  D,  which  is  similarly 
dominant  to  species  types  A,  B,  and  (',  again  is  accompanied  by  further 
specialization  toward  a  nearly  total  use  of  plant  material  as  food.  Judging 
by  the  results  of  field  studies  presented  earlier  in  this  paper,  the  red- 
backed  mouse,  Clethrionunujs,  represents  a  species  at  the  D  level.  In  most 
situations  where  both  it  and  Peromyscus  are  present,  Clethrionamys  domi- 
nates. Following  the  previous  line  of  reasoning  we  may  anticipate  that 
such  a  D-type  species  emits  vocalizations  having  not  only  the  basic  char- 
acteristics of  species  A,  B,  and  C,  but,  in  addition,  possesses  vocalization 
characteristics  peculiar  to  itself. 

At  each  level  a  species  responds  not  only  to  its  own  vocalizations,  but 
also  to  those  of  all  species  ranked  above  it,  as  if  they  were  emitted  by  its 
own  kind.  The  lower  the  rank  of  a  species,  the  greater  are  the  number  of 
vocal  stimuli  to  which  it  responds  and  thus  the  smaller  its  home  range. 
Reasoning  back  from  present  day  simall-mammal  communities  to  the 
probable  course  of  evolution,  it  appears  that  there  is  a  correlation  between 
(a)  social  rank  in  the  domination  of  the  use  of  space,  and  (b)  the  shift 
from  carnivorous  to  herbivorous  diet.  I  do  not  believe  that  an  herbivorous 
diet  per  se  contributes  in  any  way  to  social  dominance.  Rather,  it  has 
relevance  only  because  of  the  later  development  of  flowering  plants  and 
grasses.  Evolution  of  more  advanced  types  of  plant  permitted  evolution  of 
small  mammals  specialized  to  utilize  these  new  resoiu'ces.  To  a  certain 
degree  such  feeding  specialization  would  enable  an  incipient  species  to 
avoid  direct  competition  with  its  progenitor.  Once  removed  from  overt 
competition  with  its  progenitor,  psychological  dominance  by  the  incipient 
species  could  then  proceed  through  the  process  of  increased  complexity  of 
vocalization  and  development  of  a  more  effective  filter. 

A  major  aspect  of  my  thesis  is  that  psychological  dominance,  resulting 
from  a  greater  complexity  of  vocalization  and  an  increased  effectiveness 
of  the  neural  filter,  far  outweighs  all  niche  specializations  in  determining 
the  relative  abundance  of  species  comprising  the  small  mammal  commimity. 


1.   The  Social  Use  of  Space  75 

Such  psychological  dominance  also  develops  among  members  of  a  single 
species,  at  least  at  the  higher  levels  of  the  interspecific  hierarchy.  We  have 
already  presented  the  data  and  logic  which  gave  rise  to  the  concept  of 
constellation  formation.  On  the  average,  each  constellation  has  one  central 
alpha  member  with  a  very  large  home  range,  six  beta  members  with  home 
ranges  slightly  smaller  than  for  alpha  individuals,  and  five  gamma  members 
with  markedly  restricted  home  ranges.  Such  intraspecific  differences  in 
home  range  size  suggests  that,  among  alpha  (C  or  D  types  in  above  dis- 
cussion) species  at  least,  developmental  alterations  in  vocalization  arise. 
Gamma  members  of  alpha  species  exhibit  a  minimum  complexity  of 
vocalization.  Successively  beta  and  alpha  members  increase  the  complexity 
of  their  vocalizations.  All  members  of  the  species  presumably  have  the 
same  level  of  filter  development.  Even  so,  the  differences  in  complexity  of 
vocalization  should  result  in  alpha  members  mainly  ignoring  vocalizations 
of  beta  and  gamma  individuals  insofar  as  these  signals  have  a  negative 
valance.  Beta  members  respond  not  only  to  other  beta  individuals  but  to 
their  alpha  associates.  Gamma  members  not  only  treat  the  vocalizations 
of  other  gamma  members  as  inhibitory  stimuli,  but  are  likewise  similarly 
influenced  by  those  from  their  beta  and  alpha  associates. 

It  must  be  borne  in  mind  that  this  whole  discussion  of  vocalizations  and 
filters  in  the  context  of  the  small  mammal  community  is  strictly  theoretical. 
However,  it  not  only  provides  a  conceptual  framework  offering  one  inter- 
pretation of  empirical  data,  but  also  enables  formulations  capable  of  ex- 
perimental analysis. 

A  study  of  the  complexity  and  intensity  of  vocalizations  is  suggested  as 
having  priority  in  testing  the  theory.  Sorex,  Blarina,  Peromijscus,  and 
Clethrionomys,  respectively  representing  theoretical  types  A,  B,  C,  and  D 
discussed  above,  should  serve  as  particularly  useful  subjects,  especially 
since  they  all  may  occur  in  the  same  small-mammal  community.  The  vocali- 
zation of  each  higher  member  of  the  series  should  include  the  basic  char- 
acteristics of  all  lower  ones  and  in  addition  possess  characteristics  not  held 
by  lower  members  in  the  series.  Furthermore,  if  each  of  these  species  is 
experimentally  exposed  to  a  conditioned  avoidance  situation  where  vocali- 
zations of  their  own  species  serve  as  a  conditioned  stimulus,  one  may 
anticipate  that  vocalizations  of  the  other  members  of  the  series  will  equally 
well  induce  avoidance  upon  their  replacement  of  the  intraspecific  stimulus 
only  if  the  vocalization  represents  a  higher  member  of  the  series.  Unfortu- 
nately, the  extreme  paucity  of  om*  knowledge  of  vocalizations  of  small 
mammals  necessitates  these  suggestions  in  lieu  of  any  firm  experimental 
evidence. 

Development  of  cryptic  behavior  forms  an  ancillary  aspect  of  this  con- 
cept. The  lower-ranked  shrews  typically  spend  much  of  their  time  in  under- 


76  John  B.  Calhoun 

ground  runways.  Presumably  such  behavior  enables  these  species  to  reduce 
the  frequency  or  intensity  that  they  experience  by  the  vocalizations  of 
their  dominant  associates.  When  Dr.  Barbehenn  first  joined  me  in  these 
studies  of  small-mammal  communities  he  insisted  that  the  only  effective 
way  of  trapping  Blarina  was  to  set  traps  at  points  where  excavations  in 
humus  revealed  underground  runways.  However,  it  had  been  my  experi- 
ence with  continuous  removal  trapping,  where  traps  were  set  at  fixed 
intervals  from  stations  without  any  regard  to  underground  runways, 
that  once  the  dominant  Peromyscus  has  been  removed  Blarina  were  caught 
on  surface  sets  with  equal  ease  as  earlier  for  Peromyscus.  Therefore,  when 
he  initiated  his  Chadwick  Woods  study  (Fig.  10)  he  set  his  traps  on  the 
surface  without  regard  to  runways.  During  the  initial  days  while  many 
Peromyscus  still  survived,  most  Blarina  were  taken  only  by  traps  acci- 
dentally set  by  underground  runways.  As  the  number  of  Peromyscus 
became  fewer  and  fewer  by  the  removal  trapping,  not  only  were  more 
Blarina  caught,  but  also  an  increasing  percentage  of  these  had  so  entered 
traps  as  to  indicate  clearly  that  they  had  been  wandering  about  the  surface 
and  were  not  emerging  through  the  leafmold  below  the  trap.  Thus,  a  re- 
duction in  crypticism  accompanied  enlargement  of  home  range.  This  same 
change  in  behavior  characterizes  the  typically  subterraneous  mouse  Pitymys 
following  removal  of  its  dominant  associates.  The  prior  discussion  of  the 
special  case  of  both  bodily  movement  and  vocalizations  of  another  in- 
dividual become  important  here.  We  might  thus  expect  that  vocalization 
of  shrews  would  become  reduced  as  they  become  more  cryptic  in  the 
presence  of  dominant  mice.  Also  as  mice  are  removed  from  the  habitat, 
shrews  should  not  only  spend  more  time  out  on  the  surface,  but  they 
should  vocalize  more. 

This  section  cannot  be  closed  without  brief  reference  to  the  meadow 
mouse  Microtus.  Data  from  a  recent  unpublished  study  by  Dr.  Barbehenn 
in  an  abandoned  orchard  in  Maryland  suggest  that  this  genus  represents 
one  terminal  phase  in  this  evolution  of  types,  which  on  the  psychological 
level  enables  successively  evolved  types  to  acquire  a  more  dominant  posi- 
tion with  reference  to  the  use  of  space.  At  points  isolated  from  each  other 
by  at  least  600  feet  he  placed  covered  feeding  stations  which  contained 
rolled  oats  mixed  with  dyes  which  stained  the  fur  of  animals  eating  there. 
Daily  removal  of  food  increased  for  nearly  a  month,  after  which  the  daily 
removal  fluctuated  about  an  asymptote  for  another  month.  At  the  end  of 
this  time  removal  trapping  was  conducted  along  a  circular  trapline  having 
a  radius  of  150  feet  from  the  feeding  station.  Practically  all  the  Peromyscus 
taken  were  marked  with  the  red  dye,  but  no  Microtus  were  so  marked. 
This  indicated  that  Microtus  living  at  this  distance  from  the  feeding  sta- 
tions were  not  only  unaffected  by  it,  but  their  home  ranges  were  unaffected 


1.  The  Social  Use  of  Space  77 

by  Perovnjscus,  even  those  living  much  farther  out  in  the  habitat,  altering 
their  movement  and  passing  with  increasing  frequency  and  in  an  altered 
pattern  through  the  home  ranges  of  Microtus.  This  trapping  was  followed 
by  additional  removal  trapping  along  several  concentric  circular  traplines 
between  the  original  one  and  the  bait  station.  Many  more  Microtus  were 
trapped,  but  practically  all  Peromyscus  had  been  removed  in  the  initial 
trapping.  Furthermore,  the  drop-off  in  catch  of  Microtus  in  this  later 
trapping,  as  the  concentric  lines  came  nearer  the  initial  one,  indicated  that 
only  Microtus  living  within  37  feet  of  the  initial  circular  line  had  been  taken 
by  it.  It  furthermore  indicated  that  Microtus  were  not  wandering  about, 
as  we  had  supposed  they  might  be,  but  regardless  of  distance  from  the 
feeding  stations  had  maintained  fixed  home  ranges  despite  many  strange 
Peromyscus  focusing  their  movements  toward  this  one  central  spot.  Paren- 
thetically, it  might  be  added  that  this  altered  behavior  by  Peromyscus  did 
disrupt  Sorex  home  ranges.  They  were  taken  in  relatively  large  number 
beginning  with  the  very  first  day  of  trapping  along  the  initial  circular 
trapline. 

Now  the  question  arises,  "What  do  these  data  indicate?"  Since  Peromys- 
cus altered  its  behavior  by  readjusting  its  movements  toward  the  feeding 
station  as  if  Microtus  was  not  there,  I  conclude  that  one  of  Microtus'^ 
adaptations  has  been  the  loss  of  vocalization.  On  the  other  hand,  since 
Microtus  appears  to  be  unaffected  by  the  altered  movements  of  Peromyscus 
and  the  periodic  concentration  of  many  Peromyscus  in  one  spot,  which  must 
increase  the  vocalizations  at  that  spot,  I  conclude  that  Microtus  has 
evolved  a  filter  system  so  effective  that  they  can  ignore  signals  from  other 
species.  This  leads  me  to  suspect  that  Microtus  represents  what  might  be 
called  a  secondary  herd-type  species.  See  later  discussion  on  the  evolution 
of  colonialism  and  herding. 


X.  Psychological  Dominance  as  the  Primary  Component  of  the  Niche 

Hutchinson  (1957)  includes  the  relationships  one  species  has  with  others 
as  comprising  aspects  of  its  niche  requirements  equally  to  be  considered 
along  with  food,  shelter,  and  climatic  factors.  Implicit  in  his  inclusion  of 
interspecific  social  factors  in  niche  characterization  is  the  relative  capacity 
for  any  two  given  species  to  compete  for  some  given  environmental  com- 
modity or  condition.  The  more  nearly  the  identity  of  their  nonsocial  niche 
requirements,  the  more  important  become  social  relationships  both  as  a 
component  of  the  niche  and  in  leading  to  the  elimination  of  one  species  by 
the  other  via  the  principle  of  "competitive  exclusion"  (Hardin,  1960). 

In  terms  of  this  viewpoint  and  the  marked  difference  in  diet,  morphology. 


X 


78  John  B.  Calhoun 

and  behavior  of  such  distantly  related  genera  as  Cleihrionomys,  Peromyscus, 
Blarina,  and  Sorex,  we  would  suspect  minimum  niche  similarities  and  thus 
little  influence  of  one  genus  upon  the  other. 

If  their  niche  reciuirements  are  really  quite  dissimilar,  if  within  any 
individual's  home  range  each  requirement  is  represented  at  many  points, 
and  if  in  most  home  range-sized  plots  most  of  the  niche  requirements  for 
each  genus  abundantly  occur,  then,  depending  upon  the  intraspecific 
factors  leading  to  fluctuations  in  density,  we  might  in  fact  anticipate  that 
one  or  several  of  these  genera  might  occur  simultaneously  in  high  densities. 
In  fact,  observed  relative  densities  do  behave  in  such  a  fashion.  Were  we 
content  to  rest  our  case  solely  on  relative  densities,  we  would  remain  con- 
tent with  the  satisfactoriness  of  such  logic  in  revealing  true  relationships. 

However,  the  time  sequence  analysis  of  removal  captures  leads  to  a 
formulation  of  community  dynamics  suggesting  the  nearly  complete 
invalidity  of  the  concept  of  lack  of  influence  of  one  species  upon  another  if 
their  niche  reciuirements  markedly  differ.  In  fact,  the  concept  of  competition 
has  little  relevance.  Instead,  "home  range  inhibition"  becomes  the  most 
useful  concept.  Home  range  inhibition  is  the  consequence  of  processes 
through  which  the  individual  reacts  to  a  signal  as  if  it  were  identical  to  a 
different  signal  with  which  it  shares  certain  physical  characteristics.  In 
other  words,  an  animal  may  react  to  a  signal  emitted  by  a  member  of  an- 
other species  as  if  it  were  the  signal  emitted  by  another  member  of  its  own 
species  with  whom  its  interactions  have  led  to  its  characterizing  the  signal 
as  noxious.  Such  a  species  becomes  a  subordinate  member  of  the  community. 

Signals  of  the  dominant  species  must  contain  not  only  the  basic  char- 
acteristics of  the  subordinate  but  some  characteristics  peculiar  to  itself. 
Thus,  when  both  a  dominant  and  a  subordinate  species  occur  simultane- 
ously in  a  habitat,  members  of  the  dominant  species  will  come  to  associate 
no.xious  qualities  only  to  that  portion  of  the  signal  which  is  species-specific 
since  it  is  the  only  portion  of  the  signal  which  always  accompanies  a  nega- 
tive intraspecific  interaction.  If  we  consider  a  commiuiitj^  composed  of 
four  species  A,  B,  C,  and  D  (following  the  nomenclature  of  Section  IX) 
in  which  D  is  most  dominant  and  A  most  subordinate,  and  signal  compo- 
nents a,  b,  c,  and  d  are  observed,  then  Z)'s  signal  should  include  components 
a,  b,  c,  and  d;  C's  should  include  a,  b,  and  c;  5's  only  a,  and  b;  and  A's 
only  a. 

Even  though  A  learns  to  define  signal  a  as  noxious  only  by  reacting  with 
other  A  associates,  he  will  respond  similarly  to  detection  of  the  a  compo- 
nents of  his  B,  C,  and  D  associates.  I  have  cited  data  indicating  that  the 
greater  the  frec^uency  of  rele^'ant  stimuli  in  the  environment  the  greater 
will  be  the  probability  of  the  origin  of  a  neural  signal  leading  to  termination 
of  the  behavior  of  outward  movement  from  home.  In  this  situation,  every 


1.  The  Social  Use  of  Space  79 

time  A  emerges  and  starts  on  an  outward  excursion  ho  experiences  a  bom- 
bardment by  so  many  a  signals  that  his  trip  is  shortly  terminated  and  he 
returns  home.  In  a  similar  fashion,  5's  home  range  will  become  contracted 
but  not  so  much  as  A's  because  A  does  not  emit  h  signals,  whose  sum  at 
any  one  time  is  contributed  to  only  by  species  B,  C,  and  D.  C  will  have 
only  a  slightly  inhibited  home  range  since  it  is  responsive  only  to  signals 
contributed  by  itself  and  D.  At  the  apex  of  the  system  members  of  species 
D  are  influenced  only  by  d. 

At  the  psychological  apex  of  the  community,  further  differentiation 
occurs  among  the  members  of  the  dominant,  D  species.  According  to  the 
formulation  developed  in  Section  VIII,  C,  members  of  this  species  have  the 
capacity  to  differentiate  into  alpha,  beta,  and  gamma  members.  Their 
basic  c?-type  signal  becomes  differentiated  into  (/„,  dis,  and  dy  components. 
Alpha  individuals  possess  all  three;  beta  members  only  d^  and  dy]  while 
gamma  members  emit  only  dy. 

This  purely  theoretical  formulation  predicts  that  home  range  size  and 
complexity  of  signals  emitted  are  positively  correlated.  The  observed 
data  on  removal  captures  lead  to  inferences  of  home  range  expansion 
following  removal  of  associates.  Intra-  and  interspecific  inhibitions  are  so 
apparent  as  to  demand  the  minimum  assumptions  made  above  for  main- 
taining such  a  complex  spatial  organization  of  the  small-mammal  com- 
munity. Compare  these  formulations  with  those  of  "velocity"  developed 
in  Sections  XIII,  A;  XIV,  A;  and  XIV,  E  and  F.  Such  comparison  leads 
to  the  conclusion  that  the  greater  an  individual's  velocity,  the  more  com- 
plex will  be  the  pattern  of  signals  he  emits. 

Psychological  dominance  is  then  the  ability  to  inhibit  the  home  range  of 
others  resulting  from  the  fact  that  the  dominant  shares  certain  signal 
characteristics  with  the  subordinate,  but  in  addition  possesses  signals 
which  the  subordinate  lacks.  The  sharing  of  a  signal  by  a  species  with 
another  species  which  it  usually  dominates  may  lead  to  mutual  inhibition 
of  home  range  or  actual  reversal  of  roles. 

Consider  the  Clethrionomijs  and  Peromyscus  relationship.  The  greater 
frequency  with  which  Clethrionomijs  appears  to  be  dominant  to  Peromyscus 
suggests  that  it  has  the  c-rZ-type  signal  while  Peromyscus  has  only  c.  We 
may  ignore  other  shared  characteristics  of  their  signals.  In  fact,  we  may 
focus  only  on  the  shared  c  component.  Chance  vagaries  of  the  system  may 
from  time  to  time,  after  a  crash  in  the  populations  of  both  species,  result  in 
a  marked  preponderance  of  Peromyscus  over  Clethrionomys.  Similarly,  at 
the  southern  periphery  of  its  range,  we  can  expect  Peromyscus  frequently 
to  be  more  dense  than  Clethrionomys.  Clethrionomys  will  then  meet  too 
infrequently  to  make  any  associations  with  their  own  species'  specific 
signal.  However,  encounters  will  occur  more  frequently  between  Clethri- 


80  John  B.  Calhoun 

onomys  and  the  more  numerous  Peromyscus.  Regardless  of  which  species 
dominates  in  the  actual  encounter,  the  interspecific  common  characteristic 
of  the  signal  may  be  expected  to  assume  only  a  negative  quality  by  Clethri- 
onomys.  Some  interactions  among  Peromyscus  must  be  of  a  positive  nature. 
Thus,  even  though  both  species  may  detect  the  interspecies  common  com- 
ponent of  their  signals  with  equal  frequency,  Peromyscus  might  be  expected 
to  exhibit  less  contraction  of  its  home  range  because  this  signal  is  less 
aversive  than  it  is  for  Clethrionomys. 

This  line  of  reasoning  applies  also  at  times  to  relationships  among  three 
species.  In  the  unusual  situation  (Fig.  12)  in  which  Blarina,  Peromyscus, 
and  Clethrionomys  were  all  c^uite  abundant,  the  seciuential  trapping  data 
clearly  indicate  that  inhibition  of  home  range  nearly  equally  characterizes 
all  three  species. 

In  such  a  system,  the  extent  and  center  of  an  animal's  home  range  de- 
pends not  so  much  on  characteristics  of  the  habitat  as  on  the  current 
density  and  origins  of  signals,  and  the  temporal  and  spatial  history  of 
interaction  among  members  of  the  community  during  the  most  recent 
generations.  The  absence  of  an  animal  in  a  locality  cannot  be  construed 
to  mean  unsuitability  of  the  habitat.  It  is  conceivably  possible  to  delineate 
microhabitat  characteristics  contributing  to  the  animal's  niche  even  under 
such  circumstances.  However,  it  requires  that  we  know  an  individual's 
home  range  center  and  that  we  mark  out  stations  along  a  circumference  of 
a  circle  having  a  radius  which  will  result  in  a  high  probability  of  the  animal 
crossing  that  circumference.  A  1-sigma  radius  should  prove  effective. 
Examination  of  stations  from  such  a  series,  which  have  a  greater  than 
chance  frequency  of  visitation,  might  lead  us  to  isolate  those  conditions 
which  do  contribute  to  an  animal's  niche. 


XL  An  Induced  Invasion 

The  major  portion  of  this  section  presents  the  author's  interpretation  of 
a  study  conducted  by  Webb  and  Rosasco  (1953).  It  describes  the  response 
of  the  red-backed  mice,  Clethrionomys,  surrounding  a  30-acre  tract  within 
which  continuous  removal  trapping  was  conducted  for  80  consecutive 
days.  In  this  account  it  will  become  apparent  that  the  concepts  elaborated 
remain  inconclusive.  Even  so,  their  implications,  when  taken  in  conjunc- 
tion with  the  other  sections  of  this  paper,  warrant  their  presentation. 

A  brief  history  of  events  leading  up  to  Dr.  Webb's  study  provides  a 
background  for  appreciating  the  objective.  During  the  summer  of  1950, 
while  I  was  in  residence  at  the  Roscoe  B.  Jackson  Memorial  Laboratory 
as  a  National  Institute  of  Mental  Health  Special  Fellow,  Dr.  A.  Dexter 


1.  The  Social  Use  of  Space  81 

Hinckley,  then  a  sophomore  at  Yale  University,  was  assigned  to  me  for 
supervision  on  a  research  project.  I  assigned  him  the  task  of  placing  eight 
NACSM  traplines  in  an  80-acre  central  portion  of  a  much  more  extensive 
tract.  He  ran  the  480  traps  for  15  consecutive  days.  At  that  time  it  was 
generally  accepted  that  if  one  plotted  a  regression  line  through  points 
representing  catch  per  day  as  a  function  of  total  prior  catch,  this  line  would 
intersect  the  abscissa  at  a  point  denoting  the  total  population  (see  Calhoun 
and  Casby,  1958,  pp.  15-16  for  a  summary  of  this  procedure).  Previously, 
Hayne  (1949)  had  made  such  estimations  on  the  basis  of  3  days  of  consecu- 
tive trapping  as  employed  by  the  North  American  Census  of  Small  Mam- 
mals. Thus,  it  was  my  anticipation  that  Hinckley's  longer-term  trapping 
would  merely  result  in  the  anticipated  continuous  decline  from  day  to  day, 
and  that  by  the  15th  day  essentially  no  more  animals  would  be  entering 
the  traps.  Furthermore,  by  the  end  of  the  loth  day  the  total  should  ap- 
proximate that  predicted  by  the  intersection  of  the  abscissa  by  the  regres- 
sion line  as  noted  above.  During  the  first  3  days  the  catch  per  day  did 
decline.  However,  on  the  fourth  day  Hinckley  reported  a  larger  catch  than 
on  the  first  day.  Well,  I  thought  this  was  just  due  to  the  vagaries  of  chance 
or  some  unrecognized  climatic  factor.  On  every  successive  day  his  report 
was  the  same — more  animals  than  yesterday.  On  the  15th  day,  three  times 
as  many  animals  were  taken  as  on  the  first  day.  All  of  this  was  very 
perturbing. 

After  several  months  of  reflection  I  came  up  with  this  formulation: 
During  the  first  3  days  the  number  of  residents  in  the  80-acre  tract  had 
been  drastically  reduced.  Animals  at  the  periphery  of  this  tract  would  then 
find  themselves  with  the  normal  number  of  neighbors  centrifugal  to  the 
trapped  area  but  with  very  few  remaining  centripetally.  Now,  suppose 
that  in  the  normal  state  maintenance  of  a  uniform  distribution  through  the 
environment  is  facilitated  by  vocalizations  and  audition.  Each  individual's 
customary  state  would  be  that  of  detecting  an  equal  intensity  of  signals  in 
all  directions  from  the  center  of  its  home  range.  In  these  terms  mice  and 
shrews  at  the  periphery  of  the  trapped  area  would  detect  few  signals 
toward  it  but  comparatively  many  away  from  it.  Their  normal  response 
being  to  move  so  as  to  e(iualize  signals  coming  from  all  directions,  their 
response  to  the  neighboring  depleted  area  would  be  to  move  in  toward  it. 
They  then  became  exposed  to  traps  still  set  in  the  central  area  and  many 
of  these  invaders  were  killed.  This  left  the  next  peripheral  group  of  animals 
in  the  same  situation  so  that  they  also  began  moving  inward  toward  the 
trapped  depleted  area.  By  this  process,  a  chain  reaction  was  set  in  motion 
in  which  the  entire  population  for  a  great  distance  from  the  trapped  area 
began  moving  toward  it.  If  we  visualize  the  trapped  area  as  a  circle,  rather 
than  its  actual  rectangular  shape,  it  becomes  apparent  that  if  animals  are 


82  John  B.  Calhoun 

moving  in  at  a  constant  speed  of  tra\'el  per  day,  those  invading  the  trapped 
area  each  day  will  represent  residents  from  successive  bands  of  equal 
width.  Each  successive  day  the  majority  of  the  captures  will  be  from  a 
more  distant  band  than  on  the  prior  day.  In  a  system  of  concentric  bands 
of  equal  width,  each  band  more  distant  from  the  center  contains  a  larger 
area  than  the  next  innermost  one.  Thus,  with  density  proportional  to  area 
and  invasion  into  the  central  area  transpiring  from  a  constant  rate  of  move- 
ment toward  it,  catch  per  day  should  increase  with  time. 

At  the  time  when  this  formulation  was  just  crystallizing  in  the  fall  of 
1950,  Dr.  Webb  wrote  me  concerning  their  developing  plans  for  long- 
range  studies  of  small-mammal  populations  of  the  Huntington  Forest  near 
Newcomb,  New  York.  I  mentioned  the  interesting  results  obtained  by 
Hinckley,  and  the  hypothesis  generated  by  them.  His  response  was  to 
replicate  Hinckley's  study. 

Details  of  these  two  studies  have  been  included  here  (Figs.  12  and  13A). 
However,  at  the  time  of  these  two  studies  the  failure  of  catch  per  day  to 
decline  over  time  blinded  us  to  the  story  which  the  differences  of  input 
for  different  genera  could  tell  us.  That  is,  it  was  not  realized  at  that  time 
that  expansion  of  home  range  by  subordinate  members  of  the  dominant 
species  and  by  all  members  of  the  remaining  subordinate  species  could 
lead  to  results  by  removal  trapping  in  which  catch  per  day  did  not  decline 
even  though  there  were  no  invasions.  Xot  recognizing  this  possibility  the 
results  of  these  two  studies  were  described  (Calhoun  and  Webb,  1953)  as 
supporting  the  hypothesis  that  continuous  removal  trapping  did  in  fact 
lead  to  invasion  of  the  trapped-out  area  by  residents  from  surrounding 
areas. 

By  that  time  I  had  become  associated  with  the  Neuropsychiatry  Divi- 
sion of  the  Walter  Reed  Army  Institute  for  jMedical  Research.  If  this 
hypothesis  were  correct  it  might  have  relevance  both  to  lemming  migration 
(Elton,  1942)  and  to  certain  panic  phenomena  of  troops  (Ranson,  1949; 
Caldwell,  et  al.,  1951).  In  order  to  explore  this  phenomenon  in  more  detail, 
it  was  possible  for  Dr.  Webb  to  negotiate  Contract  Number  DA-49-007- 
j\ID-325  between  the  ^Medical  Research  and  Development  Board  (Office  of 
the  Surgeon  General,  Department  of  the  Army)  and  the  College  of  Forestry-, 
State  University  of  New  York.  This  enabled  him  and  his  associate  to  execute 
a  large  series  of  studies  on  the  response  of  small  mammals  to  removal  trap- 
ping. The  major  details  of  these  studies  will  be  published  elsewhere  by  Dr. 
Webb.  I  merely  wish  here  to  present  a  brief  outhne  of  one  of  these  studies 
because  of  its  importance  to  the  general  theme  being  developed  in  this 
paper. 

In  the  center  of  an  extensive  forested  tract  they  established  a  circular 
trapline  with  a  562-foot  radius.  Along  this  trapline  781  snap-traps  were 


1.   The  Social  Use  of  Space 


83 


placed,  one  at  approximately  every  .5  feet.  Along  two  diameters  at  right 
angles  to  each  other,  156  additional  traps  were  placed,  3  to  a  station  and 
with  a  50-foot  interval  between  stations.  This  central  cross  of  traps  was 
intended  to  facilitate  removal  of  residents  and  afterward  capture  any  in- 
vaders "filtering"  through  the  peripheral  circular  trapline.  All  937  traps 
were  run  for  80  consecutive  days.  Xo  Clethrionomys  entered  the  traps  on 
the  21st  day.  Prior  to  that,  101  were  caught  and,  in  addition,  six  died  in  a 
preceding  period  of  live  trapping.  These  107  individuals  presumably 
represent  the  majority  of  residents.  Between  the  21st  and  80th  day  of 
trapping,  501  additional  redbacks  entered  traps.  Furthermore,  these  501 
represented  four  fairly  definite  waves,  roughly  15  days  elapsing  between 
the  beginning  of  each  wave  and  the  start  of  the  next  one.  Each  later  wave 
exceeded  in  numbers  that  of  the  preceding  one  (Fig.  31). 

Let  us  now  return  to  a  consideration  of  the  hypothesis,  originally  con- 
ceived from  reflecting  upon  the  results  of  Hinckley's  1950  Maine  study. 
Although  it  is  now  recognized  that  the  hypothesis  does  not  apply  to  the 
study  from  which  it  originated,  w^e  shall  now  ask  the  question:  "Does  it 


140 


UJ 

O     130 


o 


21-35  36-50  51-65  66-80 

PERIOD  OF   CAPTURE   (DAYS) 


Fig.  31.  Invasion  of  redbacked  mice  into  an  area  from  which  most  of  the  residents 
had  presumably  been  removed. 


84  John  B.  Calhoun 

apply  to  the  present  data  with  regard  to  captures  following  presumed 
removal  of  all  residents  during  the  first  20  days?"  If  it  does,  we  shall  also 
wish  to  know  whether  the  apparent  waves  might  reflect  some  basic  property 
of  intraspecific  social  organization. 

The  first  problem  concerns  determination  of  the  area  inhabited  by 
those  animals  taken  during  the  first  20  days.  It  must  include  all  the  rr^ 
area  delimited  by  the  trapline.  Furthermore,  animals  from  some  distance 
outward  from  the  trapline  must  also  have  been  caught.  On  first  thought, 
it  might  be  logical  to  anticipate  that  all  mice  whose  home  range  centers 
lay  within  3.0  home  range  sigma  of  the  circular  trapline,  and  away  from  it, 
would  be  the  only  ones  to  exposed.  However,  as  we  shall  see,  the  distance 
outward  from  the  trapline  to  which  animals  are  affected  by  it  more  likely 
approximates  the  maximum  distance  at  which  they  can  perceive  signals 
from  other  mice. 

But,  this  is  getting  ahead  of  the  analysis.  For  the  present  let  us  assume 
that  each  wave  of  mice  entering  the  traps  represents  the  inhabitants  of  a 
band  of  width  w.  Furthermore,  assume  that  during  the  first  20  days  mice 
from  a  band  of  width,  w,  immediately  outward  from  the  trapline,  get 
caught  by  it  in  addition  to  those  internal  to  the  circular  trapline. 

The  radius  from  the  center  of  the  area  being  trapped  to  the  trapline  was 
562  feet.  Thus,  the  area  sampled  during  the  first  20  days  equals 
7r(562  +  w)^.  Since  each  wave  of  invaders  is  presumed  to  represent  a  band 
of  equal  width,  w,  then  the  entire  area  sampled  during  the  entire  80  days 
equals  7r(o62  +  ow)-.  One  hundred  and  seven  mice  were  taken  from  the 
central  area,  and  608  from  the  total  area.  Thus,  to  the  extent  that  number 
of  mice  is  proportional  to  the  area  they  inhabit,  5.626  as  many  mice  in- 
habited the  total  area  as  the  central  area.  It  follows  that: 

5.626[x(562  +  w)^]  =  7r(562  +  nwy 

Thus  w  =  302  feet. 

Radii  to  the  limits  of  the  central  area  and  the  four  successive  bands 
become  864,  1166,  1468,  1770,  and  2072  feet  (Table  IX).  From  these  the 
areas  within  the  central  area  and  the  four  bands  may  be  calculated.  These 
are  areas  as  proportions  of  the  total  within  a  circle  having  a  radius  of  2072 
feet  as  given  in  Table  IX.  These  proportions  can  then  be  utilized  to  calcu- 
late the  expected  number  of  mice  residing  within  the  central  area  or  in- 
vading it  during  later  successive  periods. 

The  assumptions  force  identity  between  observed  and  expected  for  the 
central  area,  but  not  for  the  four  bands.  However,  the  observed  catch  for 
the  four  bands  will  approximate  that  of  expected  only  to  the  extent  that 
the  formulation  is  in  harmony  with  reality.  As  may  be  seen  from  Table  IX, 
observed  and  expected  numbers  approximate  each  other  so  closely  as  to 


1.  The  Social  Use  of  Space 


85 


Table  IX 

Invasion  of  Redbacked  Mice  into  an  Area  Subjected 
TO  Continuous  Removal  Trapping 


Radius  to         Proportiou  of  total 
Location  outer  edge  area  from  which 

of  area       trapped  mice  originated 


Catch 


Expected       Observed 


Central  area, 

r  =  u)  +  562  ft. 

864 

0.174 

107 

107 

Band  No.  2 

1166 

0.145 

88 

96 

Band  No.  3 

1468 

0.183 

111 

107 

Band  No.  4 

1770 

0.227 

138 

135 

Band  No.  5 

2072 

0.271 

165 
609 

163 

1.000 

608 

make  a  test  of  significance  superfluous.  This,  of  course,  only  proves  that 
the  formulation  is  not  wrong;  it  does  not  prove  its  validity.  In  the  absence 
of  any  evidence  to  the  contrary,  the  formulation  will  be  accepted  as  ap- 
proximating reality,  and  I  will  proceed  with  exploring  further  questions. 

First,  what  about  w  =  302  feet?  Webb  and  Rosasco  (1953)  and  Patric 
(1958)  offer  considerable  evidence  indicating  that  the  home  range  sigma 
for  Clethrionomijs  must  be  close  to  50  feet.  Thus,  w  =  6. Oct.  According  to 
the  independently  arrived  at  formulation,  vocal  signals  can  be  detected 
up  to  a  distance  O.Oo-  from  the  emitter  (Fig.  18  and  Section  VII,  C).  Also, 
the  diameter  of  the  constellation  approximates  G.Oo-  (Fig.  27,  Section 
VIII,  C).  Recall  that  the  constellation  represents  the  inferred  basic  unit 
of  social  organization  among  such  animals.  Thus  all  members  of  constella- 
tions which  overlap  the  circular  line,  or  even  just  touch  it  although  the 
home  range  centers  of  their  members  all  live  peripheral  to  the  trapline, 
can  detect  a  signal  void  centripetal  to  the  trapline. 

Before  going  on,  I  would  like  to  emphasize  that  the  diameter  of  1124 
for  the  circular  trapline  was  dictated  by  the  desire  to  ensure  that  mice 
arriving  at  the  trapline  would  not  be  able  to  hear  others  across  the  void  on 
the  other  side.  In  fact,  if  302  feet  does  represent  the  threshold  distance  of 
perception,  then  the  angle  of  signal  void  facing  a  mouse  arriving  at  the 
trapline  would  be  that  angle  subtended  by  chords  of  302  feet  from  that 
point,  in  this  case  159  degrees. 

The  existence  of  constellations  assumes  prior  development  of  bonds  of 
attachment  among  its  members.  Once  the  central  cross  of  traps  and  the 
circular  trapline  had  removed  all  mice  exposed  to  it,  survivors  of  constella- 


86  John  B.  Calhoun 

tioiis  ill  coutjuit  with  the  traplinc  would  then  tend  to  move  simultaneously 
toward  it.  There  would  then  arise  the  situation  in  which  out  to  a  iv  distance 
from  the  trapUne  few  mice  remained.  At  this  time  the  mice  in  the  second 
band  would  begin  moving  more  or  less  together  after  a  lag  in  time  resulting 
from  their  attachment  to  home  and  to  each  other.  No  mouse  could  leave 
home  until  several  of  its  associates  were  ready  to  move  together.  Similarly, 
residents  of  bands  3,  4,  and  5  would  start  inward  as  soon  as  most  of  their 
neighbors  of  the  adjoining  inner  band  had  moved  w  distance  toward  the 
central  trapping  area.  In  this  way  a  "chain-reaction"  was  set  in  motion  in 
which  several  bands  simultaneously  were  moving  toward  the  central  area. 

For  the  present  this  interpretation  seems  the  most  likely  one.  Whether 
the  phenonemon  has  any  analogies  to  lemming  migrations  or  troop  panic 
need  not  concern  us  here.  Of  importance  are  the  indications  that  the  in- 
fluence of  a  signal  void  extends  for  approximately  (i.O  home  range  sigma. 
The  wavelike  nature  of  invasion  suggests  that  most  of  the  mice  from  a 
band  of  6.0  sigma  move  together.  That  they  should  do  so  is  in  harmony 
with  the  concept  of  constellation  formation  into  groups  whose  mean  size 
is  12  individuals  and  whose  spatial  diameter  is  about  6.0  home  range  sigma. 

In  the  context  of  the  present  paper  this  additional  support  of  the  con- 
cept of  a  constellation  as  a  real  phenomenon  represents  the  prime  impor- 
tance of  this  study  on  an  induced  invasion.  For  it  is  the  evolution  of  the 
constellation  and  its  later  condensation  into  the  compact  colony  way  of 
life  that  I  believe  represents  the  reason  why  a  basic  group  size  of  12  adults 
is  the  most  important  one  in  the  mammalian  series. 


XII.  Derivation  of  Compact  Colonies  from  Constellations 

Accentuation  of  the  social  bonds  among  members  of  a  constellation 
must  have  increased  reproductive  effectiveness  and  permitted  survival  of 
individuals  exhibiting  a  decreased  antagonism  toward  those  neighbors  who 
become  familiar  through  repeated  contacts.  As  the  aggressiveness  became 
reduced,  beta  and  gamma  members  gradually  shifted  their  home  range 
center  toward  that  of  their  dominant  alpha  associate  until  at  last  theirs 
coincided  with  his.  A  compact  colony  will  then  be  formed  in  which  all  in- 
dividuals share  the  same  home  range.  Furthermore,  the  aggressive  actions 
of  the  single  dominant  alpha  individual  of  a  compact  colony  serves  to 
protect  the  other  members  of  his  colony  from  intrusion  by  extracolony 
members  of  the  same  species.  Whereas  constellation  formation  may  be  ob- 
served to  take  place  within  one  or  a  few  generations  from  a  prior  uniform 
distribution  of  home  range  centers,  the  development  of  compact  colonies 
from  constellations  must  have  entailed  considerable  genetic  change  over 


1.  The  Social  Use  of  Space 


87 


sufficient  generations  to  permit  the  evolution  of  a  new  species  or  genus. 
Stages  intermediate  between  that  of  constellations  and  compact  colonies 
may  be  recognized  by  the  mean  interval  between  adjoining  home  range 
centers  decreasing  from  2  sigma  toward  zero  sigma. 

A.  Compact  Colony  Formation  in  the  Norway  Rat 

Over  a  27-month  period  I  observed  the  development  of  social  organiza- 
tion in  a  population  of  Norway  rats  confined  in  a  one-ciuarter  acre  enclosure. 
Preliminary  details  have  already  been  presented   (Calhoun,  1949,  1952), 

Table  X 
Characteristics  of  Norway  Rat  Colonies 


Mean 

Mean  weight, 

Number 

Proportion 

weight, 

nonpregnant 

Proportion 

X  young 

Colony 

of 

female 

males 

females 

of  females 

per 

rats 

(gm.) 

(gm.) 

reproducing 

female 

a 

14 

0.928 

548 

457 

0.769 

4.3 

b 

6 

0.666 

511 

449 

0.750 

3.8 

c  +  d 

11 

0.600 

500 

488 

0.428 

3.4 

e 

14 

0.642 

512 

435 

0.555 

2.3 

f 

15 

0.534 

456 

427 

0.250 

2.0 

g 

16 

0.812 

432 

413 

0.153 

0 

h 

8 

0.500 

477 

357 

0 

0 

i 

13 

0.000 

442 

— 

— 

— 

J 

10 

0.100 

448 

— 

1.0 

0 

k 

13 

0.000 

429 

— 

— 

— 

and  furthei-  details  will  shortly  be  published  (Calhoun,  1963).  By  the  end 
of  the  study  the  population  had  increased  essentially  from  a  single  repro- 
ducing female  to  120  adults.  These  formed  11  clear-cut  local  colonies 
(Table  X).  Each  colony  inhabited  a  single  burrow  or  group  of  neighboring 
boxes,  placed  below  the  surface,  to  which  the  rats  had  access  by  a  drain 
tile  from  the  surface.  Each  such  burrow  or  cluster  of  inhabited  boxes  was 
separated  from  adjacent  ones  by  an  average  distance  of  about  35  feet. 
These  are  rank-ordered  from  a  to  k  in  a  descending  order  of  social  rank. 
For  the  present  purposes  we  may  equate  social  rank  with  sex  ratio  and 
reproductive  success.  High-ranking  colonies  had  few  males  and  many 
females,  most  of  whom  successfully  reared  litters  or  were  pregnant  at  the 
time  of  terminating  the  study.  As  social  rank  of  the  colony  decreased  there 


88  John  B.  Calhoun 

gradually  ensued  a  change  toward  more  males  per  female,  and  these  females 
were  less  successful  in  reproduction.  At  the  lowest  level  a  colony  consisted 
of  only  males,  or  if  females  occurred  they  were  essentially  asexual  with 
regard  to  reproductive  effectiveness.  The  lower  the  colony's  rank,  the  lower 
the  mean  weight. 

Members  of  each  colony  represented  more  than  one  place  of  birth  in  the 
pen.  Members  of  the  highest-ranked  colony,  a,  mostly  still  lived  at  the 
place  of  their  birth  or  had  come  there  from  adjacent  colonies.  As  social 
rank  of  the  colony  decreased,  its  members  represented  ever  more  different 
places  of  birth.  Despite  this  disparity  in  places  of  birth  characterizing  each 
colony,  the  total  number  of  adults  forming  each  colony  varied  little  from 
the  mean  number  of  12  characterizing  the  loosely  knit  constellation  of  such 
simple  social  types  as  Peromyscus  and  Clethrionomys. 

At  this  point  we  may  consider  possible  events  which  fostered  the  evolu- 
tion of  the  compact  colony  from  the  loosely  knit  constellation.  The  major 
impact  of  the  constellation  way  of  living  is  that  the  most  frequent  group 
size  would  be  12  individuals.  It  might,  therefore,  be  anticipated  that 
evolutionary  processes  would  adjust  the  physiology  and  behavior  of  such 
species  to  be  most  effective  and  appropriate  to  interactions  transpiring  in 
such  a  sized  group.  In  accordance  with  the  conservatism  of  evolution  we 
might  anticipate  restriction  of  group  size  about  this  optimum  of  12  as 
other  factors  caused  the  constellation  to  contract  into  a  compact  colony. 
Restriction  of  location  of  food  stands  is  the  most  likely  candidate  for  an 
appropriate  environmental  change.  Provided  the  location  of  food  became 
restricted  but  abundant  and  relatively  permanent  at  these  locations,  we 
could  expect  types  like  the  Norway  rat  to  develop.  They  build  burrows 
not  too  far  away  from  such  spatially  restricted  sources  of  food  and  all 
members  of  the  colony  participate  in  transporting  this  food  into  the  burrow 
where  large  caches  are  formed. 

Compact  colony  evolution  produced  a  situation  which  necessitated 
further  evolution  of  the  nervous  system.  If  groups  of  12  adults  assembled, 
either  as  a  consequence  of  being  born  at  the  same  place  or  from  random 
mixing,  a  more  nearly  equal  sex  ratio  would  characterize  most  colonies 
than  was  the  case  in  my  study  within  the  experimental  enclosure.  This 
presents  the  opportunity  for  development  of  marked  aggression  among 
males  and  consequent  stress  experienced  by  associated  females.  Actually 
this  was  the  initial  situation  always  characterizing  an  incipient  colony  of 
Norway  rats.  In  the  process  one  or  more  males  were  driven  off  and  suffi- 
cient females  remained,  although  some  always  left  to  keep  the  colony  at 
near  12  individuals.  The  rejected  males  either  joined  another  developing 
colony  lacking  a  male  as  dominant  as  the  one  at  the  colony  they  left  or 
they  joined  to  form  an  all-male  colony.  Where  there  was  a  single  very 


1.  The  Social  Use  of  Space  89 

dominant  male  (as  in  colony  a,  Table  X)  his  actions  kept  away  most  other 
males  at  times  when  his  harem  females  were  in  estrus.  This  reduced  the 
stress  experienced  by  females  in  estrus,  such  as  followed  the  thousands  of 
mountings  or  attempted  mountings  experienced  by  females  in  colonies 
lacking  such  dominant  males.  In  this  sex-ratio  restructuring  of  the  popula- 
tion, a  few  colonies  contribute  most  of  the  young. 

Although  this  readjustment  ensures  the  survival  of  the  species,  it  is  not 
the  conseciuence  most  important  with  regard  to  further  social  evolution. 
Each  time  a  rat  is  excluded  from  one  colony  it  attempts  to  join  another 
aggregate  in  order  that  it  will  again  find  itself  in  a  group  of  a  size  most 
compatible  with  its  physiology.  As  soon  as  a  group  exceeds  the  optimum 
size,  some  of  its  members  are  excluded  from  it.  This  results  in  a  marked 
reshuffling  of  the  population  from  the  time  of  puberty  of  young  born  in 
one  season  up  to  the  beginning  of  the  next  breeding  season.  At  this  time 
the  population  is  relatively  stable  with  regard  to  membership  of  each 
colony. 

In  the  process  of  attaining  colony  stability,  the  social  environment  is 
in  a  constant  state  of  turmoil.  Each  individual  is  forced  to  make  many 
adjustments  to  such  changes.  It  continually  has  to  learn  new  social  rela- 
tionships. These  are  learned  so  well  that  a  group  can  shift  its  place  of  resi- 
dence over  a  sufficient  distance  that  passage  by  or  through  other  colonies 
is  necessitated.  Even  so,  they  can  maintain  their  group  integrity.  I  am 
convinced  that  the  necessity  of  making  such  changes  of  membership  from 
one  colony  to  another  required  for  reproductive  survival  of  the  species 
has  resulted  in  the  evolution  of  the  Norway  rat  into  a  species  which  is  not 
only  highly  perceptive  of  changes  within  the  environment  but  has  the 
capacity  to  learn  required  adjustments  of  behavior. 

Although  such  capacities  for  perception  and  learned  adjustment  must 
have  arisen  in  the  context  of  a  changing  social  environment,  these  same 
capacities  then  become  available  for  perceiving  and  adjusting  to  nonsocial 
changes  in  the  environment.  Among  ecologists  this  extreme  awareness  to 
changes  in  their  environment  by  Norway  rats  has  been  termed  the  "strange 
object  reaction"  (e.g.,  Chitty  and  Southern,  1954).  Among  psychologists 
it  is  reflected  in  the  studies  falling  under  the  broad  rubric  of  ''open-field 
emotional  behavior"  (e.g..  Hall,  1934,  Schneirla  and  Tobach,  19G2). 

If  I  were  to  make  my  evolving  thesis  concerning  the  social  use  of  space 
complete,  I  should  substantiate  the  role  of  vocal  communication  at  the 
compact  colony  level  of  social  evolution.  Unfortunately,  I  cannot  cite  any 
adequate  proof  regarding  the  nature  of  its  function.  Norway  rats  do  have 
a  wide  scope  of  vocalization  ranging  from  the  loud  signal  accompanying 
the  termination  of  a  fight  to  the  low  chirping  and  whining  one  can  detect 
if  one  lies  on  the  surface  of  a  burrow  with  his  ear  at  an  entrance  hole.  Many 


90  John  B.  Calhoun 

other  vocalizations  also  occiii-  when  rats  are  on  the  surface  near  their 
burrows  and  also  while  at  the  source  of  food.  I  can  only  suspect  that  among 
these  there  is  specific  communication  among  alpha  members,  the  dominant 
males,  neighboring  colonies,  and  that  rats  at  the  food  source  emit  a  signal 
which  might  be  termed  a  "here  is  food"  signal.  Unless  there  are  such  signals, 
it  is  difficult  for  me  to  understand  many  of  the  observed  behaviors  of  rats 
which  clearly  indicated  that  they  were  aware  when  other  rats  were  or  were 
not  at  the  food  source,  even  though  they  could  not  see  it. 

B.  Howler  Monkeys,  a  Compact  Colony  Living  Species 

Carpenter  (1962)  summarized  the  results  of  field  research  on  this  species 
{Alouatta  palliata)  during  the  past  thirty  years.  Tabular  data  on  136  dis- 
tinct groups  show  the  number  of  adult  males  and  females  and  the  number 
of  immature  individuals  in  each  group.  I  derived  Fig.  32  from  these  data. 
Two  large  groups  containing  27  and  31  adults,  respectively,  were  omitted 
from  the  analysis. 

Regardless  of  group  size  all  groups  contained  more  females  than  males. 
Excluded  males  live  in  a  state  of  near  isolation  and  have  very  little  associa- 
tion either  with  each  other  or  the  organized  groups.  As  with  the  Norway 
rat,  reproduction  within  compact  colonies  apparently  requires  a  reduction 
in  the  number  of  males  in  groups  for  effective  reproduction  to  take  place. 

One  of  the  central  hypotheses  in  my  formulation  of  the  social  use  of 
space  is  that  constellation  formation  must  have  served  as  a  mold  which 
so  guided  evolution  that  beha\'ior  and  physiology  would  become  fixed  so 
that  they  would  have  optimum  expression  in  a  group  of  12  adults.  For 
howler  monkeys,  groups  of  9  to  11  are  encountered  more  frequently  than 
smaller  and  larger  ones  (Curve  A,  Fig.  32).  And  although  the  decline  in 
frequency  of  groups  containing  more  than  1 1  adults  is  not  so  rapid  as  ex- 
pected (Table  \'III),  the  observed  data  do  reveal  a  marked  decrease  in 
frequency  of  larger  groups.  Furthermore,  only  3%  of  the  136  groups  ex- 
ceeded the  maximum  of  19  anticipated  by  the  theory.  The  theory  indicates 
no  expectation  of  groups  containing  less  than  7  adults,  yet  26.5%  of  the 
136  groups  of  howler  monkeys  did  contain  less  than  7  adults.  However,  it 
must  be  pointed  out  that  such  theory  presupposes  completion  of  all  social 
processes  culminating  in  a  group  having  considerable  stability  of  member- 
ship. Incipient  groups  formed  from  the  fragmentation  of  larger  ones  and 
larger  ones  approaching  the  point  where  fragmentation  is  imminent  should 
logically  both  be  excluded  in  comparing  observed  and  theoretical  fre- 
quencies of  group  sizes.  However,  present  knowledge  prevents  such  a 
comparison. 


1.  The  Social  Use  of  Space 


91 


Production  of  young  is  fairly  ineffective  in  small  groups  in  comparison 
with  those  containing  10  or  11  adults  (Curve  C,  Fig.  32).  In  general,  the 
trend  of  young  per  female  also  decreases  as  groups  get  larger  than  12  in- 
dividuals. The  two  very  large  groups  not  included  in  this  figure  and  which 
had  27  and  31  adults  were  characterized  by  only  0.18  and  0.29  young  per 
female,  respectively.  We  must  conclude  that  group  size  does  affect  those 
behaviors  and  physiology  culminating  in  the  production  and  survival  of 
young. 


UJ  ■— 

2  Q.   0.10 


P  <    0.05 
o  >. 


Vng/j 


Aa 


'''"'        o°  ^      °    °    °    °     °o      ,     o    „     -    CO 

I      1      I       I      I      I      I      I I 1 1 I I I I I 1 1 1 

5  10  15  20 

NUMBER  OF  ADULTS  IN  GROUP (HOWLER  MONKEYS) 

Fig.  32.    Group  dynamics  of  howler  monkeys  based  upon  Carpenter  (1962) . 


Another  way  of  looking  at  this  problem  is  to  ask:  "What  proportion  of 
the  total  young  are  contributed  by  each  group  size?"  For  any  group  size 
the  proportion  will  depend  upon  (a)  the  proportion  of  females  in  the 
group  (Curve  D,  Fig.  32),  (b)  the  number  of  young  per  female  (Curve  C, 
Fig.  32),  and  (c)  the  number  of  groups  of  each  size  (Curve  A,  Fig.  32); 
where  such  data  for  each  group  are  weighed  against  similar  data  for  all 
other  group  sizes.  Resultant  data  are  given  in  Curve  B,  Fig.  32.  This 
curve  clearly  shows  that  more  individuals  gain  their  initial  social  experience 


92  John  B.  Calhoun 

in  groups  of  about  the  size  anticipated  by  theory  as  most  nearly  optimum 
than  they  do  in  any  smaller  or  larger  sized  groups.  In  fact,  the  mean  number 
of  adults  with  which  the  838  young  in  these  136  groups  had  their  early 
social  experience  was  12.22!,  even  though  Curve  C,  Fig.  32,  is  flatter  than 
predicted  by  the  theory  of  Section  VIII,  D  and  Table  VIII. 

My  selection  of  the  Norway  rat  and  the  howler  monkey  as  examples  of 
compact  colony  types  is  open  to  the  criticism  that  I  selected  those  species 
which  would  support  my  theory  that  group  size  in  higher  evolved  types  is 
dependent  upon  their  evolution  from  species  which  had  been  characterized 
by  the  loose  constellation  form  of  social  use  of  space.  At  present,  this 
criticism  cannot  be  avoided.  I  wish  merely  to  say  that  my  intensive  study 
of  a  few  groups  of  Norway  rats,  and  the  extensive  study  by  Carpenter  and 
his  colleagues  of  a  large  number  of  howler  monkey  groups,  are  the  only 
ones  known  to  me  that  appear  adeciuate  for  the  present  purpose. 

All  we  can  really  say  at  present  is  that  available  data  show  that  optimum 
group  size  in  some  compact  colony  living  species  appears  to  approximate 
12  adults  and  that  this  number  is  in  harmony  with  that  number  antici- 
pated by  the  physics  of  communication  characterizing  more  primitive 
and  more  dispersed  types. 

C.  Behavioral  Sink  Development  by  the  Norway  Rat 

IVIuch  of  the  prior  sections  have  been  devoted  to  documenting  theory 
and  e\ddence  supporting  the  hypothesis  that  groups  of  12  individuals 
represent  a  major  category  of  optimum  density.  Yet  many  species  cus- 
tomarily live  as  aggregates  much  larger  than  this.  Why  such  large  groupings 
should  have  evolved  remained  a  puzzle  until  insight  developed  from  an 
unexpected  phenomenon  arising  in  the  situational  content  of  some  experi- 
mental populations  of  rats  I  was  studying  (Calhoun,  1962a). 

The  upper  portion  of  Fig.  33  presents  in  diagrammatic  fashion  the  salient 
aspects  of  the  environment  affecting  the  rats.  Four  35  square-foot  pens, 
separated  by  2-foot  high  partitions,  formed  a  linear  communication  net- 
work through  the  opportunity  of  access  between  adjoining  pens  via  the 
V-shaped  ramps,  R,  surmounting  barriers  between  pens.  F  and  W  represent 
a  superabundant  supply  of  food  and  water  in  each  pen.  "Apartment" 
houses,  H,  connected  to  the  floor  by  ramps  provided  ample  place  of  retreat 
and  rearing  young  by  most  residents.  The  //'s  of  the  left-hand  pens  I  and 
11  were  3  feet  from  the  floor,  while  in  pens  III  and  IV  a  6-foot  distance 
separated  the  i/'s  from  the  floor.  Height  formed  an  intentional  environ- 
mental factor  designed  to  produce  a  2:2:1:1  ratio  of  density  across  pens 
1:II:III:IV  as  a  consequence  of  the  inverse-proportionality-to-effort 
usage  principle. 


1.  The  Social  Use  of  Space 


93 


In  addition  the  endedness  of  the  environment  biased  movement.  After 
some  period  of  time  every  rat  tended  to  leave  the  pen  it  was  then  in  and 
go  into  an  adjoining  pen.  Rats  in  an  end  pen  could  only  go  to  the  adjoining 
center  pen,  while  a  rat  in  one  of  the  two  center  pens  could  go  to  the  other 
center  pen  or  to  the  adjoining  end  pen.  In  other  words,  when  a  rat  is  in  an 
end  pen  it  has  a  probability  of  1.0  of  going  into  the  adjacent  center  pen, 
but  if  it  is  in  a  center  pen  it  has  a  probability  of  0.5  of  going  into  the  adja- 
cent end  pen  or  0.5  of  going  into  the  other  center  pen.  Repetition  of  shifting 
by  all  members  by  the  operation  of  this  principle  alone  soon  leads  to  a 


PEN  I 

PENH 

PEN  m 

PEN  IS 

F                W 
H    3  Ft. 

==3             RC^ 

F                W 
H   3  Ft. 

H  6  Ft 

H  6  Ft 

z>  0.5 


PEN 
lor  HI 


I  ***********  *******  *' 


ASONDJFMAMJJ 
1958  MONTH  1959 

Fig.  33.  The  upper  portion  of  the  figure  represents  in  schematic  fashion  the  environ- 
ment in  which  large  social  groups  of  albino  rats  were  maintained.  See  text  for  details. 
The  lower  figure  contrasts  the  amount  of  food  consumed  in  the  most  used  and  the  least 
used  pen  from  the  inception  of  the  behavioral  sink  during  the  sixth  month  (August, 
1958)  of  the  study. 


steady  state  in  which  a  1:2:2:1  ratio  will  characterize  the  density  of  rats 
across  pens  I: II: III: IV.  See  pages  298-299  of  Calhoun  (1962a)  for  details 
of  the  mathematics  involved  in  the  origin  of  this  ratio. 

Populations  in  four  such  4-pen  environments  were  studied  from  Febru- 
ary 1958  to  July  1959.  By  the  eighth  month,  September  1958,  each  popu- 
lation consisted  of  three  generations,  each  artificially  fixed  at  near  30  in- 
dividuals. The  first  and  second  generations  were  sexually  adult,  and  the 
thu-d  was  recently  weaned.  Distribution  of  adults  at  this  time  proved  to 
be  of  considerable  importance  in  producing  a  phenomenon  I  have  termed 
a  "behavioral  sink."  Five  surveys  of  place  of  residence  gave  a  total  count 
by  pen  of  343  for  pen  I,  467  for  pen  II,  331  for  pen  III,  and  245  for  pen  IV. 


94  John  B.  Calhoun 

If  the  two  movement  biasing  principles  described  above  operated  inde- 
pendently and  equally,  the  expected  ratio  of  density  would  be  3:4:3:2 
across  pens  I: II: III: IV,  thus  giving  an  expected  distribution  for  the 
September  1958  observation  of  347:462:347:232.  The  observed  and  ex- 
pected values  are  so  nearly  the  same  as  to  support  strongly  the  belief  that 
these  two  movement  biasing  principles  were  in  fact  the  only  effective  ones 
operating  at  this  time.  These  same  factors  also  affected  the  third  generation 
then  maturing. 

Taking  mortality  into  consideration,  each  population  consisted  of  about 
80  rats  with  on  the  average  20  living  in  pen  I,  27  in  pen  II,  20  in  pen  III, 
and  13  in  pen  IV.  Only  in  pen  IV  did  the  density  approximate  the  ideal  of 
12.  Elsewhere,  particularly  in  pen  II,  density  far  exceeded  this.  From  this 
time  on  a  remarkable  change  in  the  differential  use  of  space,  particularly 
as  reflected  by  food  consumption,  set  in.  In  one  pen  (in  three  instances  it 
was  in  pen  II  and  in  the  fourth  in  pen  III)  food  consumption  increased  at 
the  expense  of  that  in  the  other  three  pens.  See  the  lower  half  of  Fig.  33. 
By  the  time  seven  months  had  elapsed,  most  rats  were  eating  all  their 
food  in  this  "favored"  location  and  all  rats  were  doing  most  of  their  eating 
there. 

The  explanation  for  this  change  appears  rather  simple.  Gnawing  food 
through  the  wire  mesh  of  the  food  hoppers  required  considerable  time.  In 
the  one  pen  w^here  more  rats  fed  than  in  the  other  pens,  the  probability  of 
two  rats  eating  side  by  side  increased.  Gradually  rats  redefined  the  eating 
situation  as  requiring  presence  of  other  rats.  Thus,  all  rats  shifted  most  of 
their  eating  to  that  pen  where  this  condition  was  most  likely  to  be  met.  It 
must  be  kept  in  mind  that  such  a  system  is  stochastic  and  not  deterministic, 
so  it  was  not  unexpected  that  pen  III  became  the  favored  place  of  eating 
by  one  of  the  four  groups.  However,  the  likelihood  of  pen  IV  ever  becoming 
the  favored  pen  is  extremely  remote. 

The  learned  need  for  social  proximity  while  engaging  in  an  act  which 
might  have  been  expressed  alone  assumed  priority  over  the  simple  original 
hunger  drive.  Food  was  not  food  without  the  presence  of  a  comrade.  This 
whole  process  of  developing  excessive  aggregations  in  order  to  satisfy  a 
secondarily  acquired  social  drive  is  what  I  mean  by  a  "behavioral  sink." 
Gradually  more  rats  also  shifted  residence  to  this  favored  place  of  eating. 
Such  behavioral  sinks  result  in  every  member  encountering  more  associates 
than  the  ideal,  and  even  more  than  necessitated  by  the  operation  of  those 
principles  of  spatially  structuring  the  environment  which  biases  movement. 

In  this  situation  marked  alterations  in  mortality  and  behavior  resulted. 
Males  became  pansexual  in  the  sense  that  they  mounted  other  rats  ir- 
respective of  sex  or  age.  Nest  building  and  maternal  behavior  became  so 
disrupted  in  most  females  as  to  preclude  the  possibility  of  most  young 


1.   The  Social  Use  of  Space  95 

surviving.  In  each  experimental  setting  the  rats  experienced  less  disturb- 
ance in  one  pen,  usually  pen  IV,  than  elsewhere,  since  they  were  somewhat 
less  trapped  in  the  behavioral  sink.  And  yet  even  here  only  half  the  young 
born  survived  to  weaning  and  their  growth  was  markedly  retarded.  In 
contrast,  in  the  pen  where  most  rats  assembled  only  half  as  many  young 
were  born  and  only  1%  of  these  survived  to  weaning. 

In  addition,  the  abnormal  frequency  of  social  interaction  resulted  in 
marked  disturbance  to  female  reproductive  physiology.  Near-term  fetuses 
died.  Some  females  with  such  dead  fetuses  shortly  succumbed  from  ap- 
parent toxemia.  Others  died  from  massive  hemorrhaging  in  many  organs, 
an  accentuation  of  the  event  likely  to  have  been  associated  with  fetal 
death.  Many  females  who  survived  such  events  later  died  as  a  conseciuence 
of  a  site  of  resorption  of  a  near-term  fetus  becoming  the  focal  point  for  the 
development  of  a  large  abscess.  Normally  death  occurred  by  the  time  the 
abscess  reached  a  diameter  of  50  mm.  As  an  example  of  this  scourge,  56% 
of  second-generation  females  died  by  a  year  of  age,  by  which  time  only 
10%  of  males  had  died. 

Were  a  species  to  survive  for  many  generations  in  an  environment 
fostering  development  of  a  behavioral  sink,  it  is  obvious  that  selection 
must  proceed  to  produce  individuals  whose  behavior  and  physiology  were 
in  harmony  with  such  a  heightened  frequency  of  social  interaction.  Wher- 
ever an  environmental  resource  which  was  formerly  so  widely  distributed 
as  to  be  readily  available  within  each  individual's  or  group's  home  range 
becomes  restricted,  then  conditions  are  ripe  for  production  of  a  behavioral 
sink.  It  is  my  belief  that  just  such  happenings  have  been  the  usual  altera- 
tions which  have  forced  the  evolution  of  horde  or  herd  type  species  from 
one  previously  characterized  by  an  optimum  group  size  of  12  adults. 


D.  Yarding  by  Deer  in  Northern  Wisconsin 

Characteristically  since  1935  white-tailed  deer  (Dahlberg  and  Guettinger, 
1956)  in  Northern  Wisconsin  assemble  during  the  winter  in  a  restricted 
area  known  as  yards.  These  cover  only  5-10%  of  their  range.  Conifers, 
which  comprise  the  major  cover  in  the  yard,  provide  protection  from  deep 
snows.  However,  such  cover  provides  only  a  secondary  quality  food.  One 
or  more  feeding  stations  were  established  in  most  yards.  During  the  1930's 
food  supplements  represented  a  small  amount  of  total  food  requirements. 
By  1953,  when  artificial  feeding  was  largely  terminated,  most  of  the  food 
reciuirements  were  supplied  at  these  feeding  stations  in  many  yards. 
Nevertheless,  many  deer  died  of  "starvation"  even  in  yards  where  the  most 
food  was  provided. 


96  John  B.  Calhoun 

The  restricted  locations  where  food  was  provided  and  the  striking  ag- 
gregations of  deer  in  their  vicinity  reaching  350  per  square  mile  strikingly 
resemble  my  experimental  populations  of  rats  from  which  developed  the 
concept  of  the  behavioral  sink.  Even  though  many  deer  died  in  the  yards, 
the  question  stands:  "Is  this  really  an  instance  of  a  behavioral  sink?" 
Gaining  insight  into  this  question  has  proved  to  be  a  difficult  detective 
job.  Shiras  (1921),  Sanders  (1939),  Swift  (1948),  Rabat  et  al.  (1953), 
Schorger  (1953),  and  Dahlberg  and  Guettinger  (1956)  proved  to  be 
particularly  helpful. 

Before  the  days  of  lumbering,  deer  were  so  scarce  in  the  primeval  forests 
of  Northern  Wisconsin  as  to  contribute  very  little  to  the  diet  of  Indians. 
Between  1860  and  1880  a  marked  increase  in  deer  followed  lumbering 
operations  with  the  consequent  development  of  openings  and  second  growth 
which  provided  abundant  food.  Although  the  deer  did  not  reach  the  den- 
sities of  1935  to  1953,  they  supported  a  major  industry  as  a  commercial 
source  of  meat.  There  is  some  mention  during  this  era  of  herds  up  to  200 
being  seen,  of  aggregations  about  salt  licks,  and  about  concentrations  in 
white  cedar  swamps  during  heavy  snows.  However,  one  gets  the  impression 
from  Schorger's  citations  that  it  was  more  customary  for  deer  to  be  scat- 
tered. After  heavy  snows  the  commercial  hunters  trailed  the  deer  until 
they  found  them  exhausted  and  trapped  by  the  deep  snow.  Schorger 
(1953,  p.  210)  writes,  "It  is  stated  by  Harvey  Braein  that  about  Christmas, 
1857,  a  crust  about  one-half  inch  in  thickness  formed  on  the  deep  snow  in 
Buffalo  County,  and  that  nearly  every  deer  perished.  Following  the  spring, 
their  bodies  were  found  in  nearly  every  coulee."  Unfortunately,  this  is  the 
sort  of  data  one  has  to  rely  on.  Even  so,  it  suggests  a  typical  pattern  of 
scattering. 

As  early  as  1920  when  the  Northern  Wisconsin  deer  herd  was  well  on 
its  way  to  recovery  after  its  prior  decimation  by  forest  fire  and  unrestricted 
hunting,  private  hunting  clubs  and  the  operators  of  tourist  camps  had  be- 
gun the  practice  of  feeding  deer  during  the  winter  months.  After  1935 
Civilian  Conservation  Corps  camps  and  the  Wisconsin  Conservation  De- 
partment greatly  increased  this  artificial  feeding.  However,  the  intensive 
artificial  feeding  characterizes  only  the  1943-1953  period.  Swift  (1948) 
states  that  yarding  had  not  conmienced  very  extensively  until  after  1941 
even  though  astonishingly  high  populations  existed  in  many  locations.  It  is 
difficult  to  escape  the  conclusion  that  the  accentuation  of  yarding  was  a 
direct  outgrowth  of  the  artificial  feeding. 

Daily  movements  rarely  exceeded  one-quarter  mile  from  the  feeding 
stations.  Thus,  available  food  outside  the  yards  remained  unutilized.  Even 
with  the  advent  of  warm  weather  deer  exhibited  considerable  reluctance 
in  leaving  the  yard  despite  increasing  new  growth  outside  it.  Even  cessa- 


1 .   The  Social  Use  of  Space  97 

tion  of  artificial  feeding  has  not  disrupted  the  marked  yarding  tendencies 
of  Wisconsin  deer.  Through  many  generations  they  have  developed  a 
culture  demanding  an  excessive  frequency  of  contact  with  others  during 
the  winter  season  when  they  would  otherwise  be  more  scattered. 

I  will  freely  grant  that  this  interpretation  of  the  origin  of  yarding  may 
be  oversimplified.  Nevertheless,  available  observations  warrant  considera- 
tion of  the  concept  of  the  behavioral  sink  as  helping  us  understand  the 
historical  development  of  yarding  to  an  excessive  degree. 


E.  Concerning  Basic  Numbers,  Nb,  for  Man 

During  the  past  half  million  years,  density  of  Homo  sapiens  has  exhibited 
a  continued  increase  over  the  inhabited  portions  of  the  earth  (Deevy, 
1960;  von  Foerster  et  al,  1960) .  Most  who  have  concerned  themselves  with 
studying  such  change  restrict  their  emphasis  to  changes  in  density  accom- 
panying advance  in  extractive  efficiency  of  natural  resources.  Less  atten- 
tion, even  by  anthropologists,  has  been  devoted  to  determining  sizes  of 
social  groupings  forming  partially  closed  systems.  Such  partially  closed 
systems  range  from  a  male-female  pair  to  that  of  a  nation  such  as  the 
United  States  among  which  interactions  among  all  members  may  be  con- 
ceived of  in  terms  of  population  potential  (Stewart,  1948;  Calhoun,  1957). 
It  will  not  be  my  purpose  here  to  treat  the  entire  range.  Rather,  I  shall 
merely  present  a  few  highly  selected  examples  of  some  of  the  smaller 
groupings  which  presumably  reflect  evolutionary  limitations  to  group 
structure.  On  the  assumption  that  these  group  sizes  represent  the  conse- 
quences of  underlying  basic  forces,  they  will  be  used  in  later  sections 
(XIII,  A  and  B)  as  data  for  developing  a  general  formulation  of  group 
size  and  social  interaction. 

For  about  98%  of  his  history  during  the  past  half-million  years,  simple 
food  gathering  limited  man's  economy  and  social  life.  One  of  the  earliest 
known  settlements  at  the  Star  Carr  site  in  east-central  England  of  nearly 
10,000  years  ago  consisted  of  five  families,  ten  adults  (Braid wood  and 
Reed,  1957) .  Thus,  it  appears  that  an  adult  group  size  not  diverging  greatly 
from  my  hypothesized  ideal  of  12  may  have  characterized  the  human 
species  up  until  at  least  10,000  years  ago. 

The  Australian  aborigines  (Birdsell,  1953,  1957)  provide  further  insight 
into  the  basic  numbers  of  human  groups.  In  terms  of  the  typical  number  of 
adults,  five  levels  are  recognizable:  (a)  the  family  with  2  adults;  (b)  the 
"horde"  or  extended  family  with  16  adults;  (c)  the  supra-horde  of  50 
adults;  (d)  the  tribe  with  200  adults;  and  (e)  the  supra-tribe  with  2200 
adults.  The  horde  forms  the  most  basic  social  group,  ranging  generally 


98  John  B.  Calhoun 

between  10  and  20  adults.  When  the  group  exceeds  20  adults  a  budding 
process  occurs,  10  adults  forming  a  minimal-sized  horde.  These  approxima- 
tions of  BirdseU's  again  suggest  a  basic  group  size  not  diverging  greatly 
from  the  12  presumably  fixed  by  much  earlier  evolution.  Occasionally, 
the  horde  may  fragment  temporarily  into  single  family  groups  when 
scarcity  of  food  demands  such  dispersal. 

Also,  occasionally  an  average  of  five  hordes,  50  adults,  may  temporarily 
assemble  into  a  supra-horde.  However,  this  grouping  appears  to  be  a  less 
basic  one  than  the  other  four.  The  third  grouping,  the  tribe,  lacks  any 
form  of  authority,  and  only  on  rare  occasions  do  the  12  or  so  hordes  forming 
the  typical  tribe  assemble.  However,  common  bonds  of  culture,  their  cus- 
toms and  value  systems,  clearly  delineate  the  tribe  as  a  social  entity. 
Furthermore,  marriages  are  primarily  restricted  to  those  between  in- 
dividuals of  different  hordes  within  the  same  tribe.  Only  in  one  local  region 
of  Australia  has  a  higher-order  grouping  evolved.  Three  supra-tribes, 
averaging  2200  adults,  represent  an  assembly  of  tribes  bound  together  by 
a  more  advanced  type  of  political  organization  characterized  by  matrilineal 
descent. 

These  data  suggest  that  cultural  evolution  has  proceeded  by  saltatorial 
steps,  each  characterized  by  some  accretion  to  the  culture.  It  wnll  be  my 
hypothesis,  to  be  developed  in  more  detail  in  Sections  XHI,  B,  2  and  B,  3, 
that  the  sole  function  of  culture  is  to  provide  a  mold  which  enables  inter- 
actions to  transpire  in  a  larger  group  such  that  their  physiological  conse- 
quences to  the  average  individual  closely  approximate  those  that  would 
result  were  the  individuals  still  living  in  a  closed  social  group  of  about  12 
individuals. 

Hallowell  (1960,  pp.  345-846)  states  that  "...  a  normative  orientation 
becomes  an  inherent  aspect  of  the  functioning  of  all  socio-cultural  systems, 
since  traditionally  recognized  standards  and  values  are  characteristic  of 
them.  Techniques  are  appraised  as  good  or  bad;  ....  Knowledge  and 
beliefs  are  judged  true  or  false.  Art  forms  and  linguistic  expression  are 
evaluated  in  relation  to  ethical  values.  All  cultures  are  infused  with  ap- 
praisals that  involve  cognitive,  appreciative,  and  moral  \'alues,"  and  "if 
the  total  ramifications  of  the  normative  orientation  of  human  societies 
are  taken  into  account,  we  have  a  major  clue  to  the  kind  of  psychological 
transformation  that  must  have  occurred  in  hominoid  evolution  w^hich 
made  this  level  of  adaptation  possible  and  some  measure  of  its  depth  and 
significance  for  an  understanding  of  the  dynamics  of  human  social  systems 
of  social  action."  Culture  so  conceived  as  normative  orientation  in  which 
individuals  play  sanctioned  roles  provides  the  structure  which  allows  in- 
dividuals to  reap  the  maximum  rewards  (the  theta,  9,  of  Section  XHI,  A) 
from  participation  in  the  social  system. 


1.   The  Social  Use  of  Space  99 

If  culture  really  does  permit  individuals  to  function  in  the  context  of  a 
larger  social  group  as  if  they  were  still  only  in  the  basic  A^'t  =  12  group  com- 
patible with  their  physiology,  then  any  disruption  in  the  culture  should 
reduce  the  group  size  since  its  unstable  state  would  then  no  longer  buffer 
the  individuals  from  the  excessive  contacts  with  their  associates.  I  am  as- 
suming that,  depending  upon  the  extent  of  the  cultural  disturbance, 
physiological  disturbances  comparable  to  those  of  my  rats  caught  in  the 
behavioral  sink  (Section  XII,  C)  would  arise. 

In  fact,  Birdsell  (1953)  demonstrates  that  such  a  phenomenon  has 
characterized  Australian  aborigines  in  recent  times.  Tribes  which  have 
recently  adopted  the  rites  of  circumcision  or  subincision  generally  have  a 
size  less  than  one-third  that  of  tribes  which  have  either  not  been  exposed 
to  these  practices  or  adopted  them  long  ago.  Furthermore,  the  historical 
records  indicate  that  tribes  once  reduced  in  numbers  after  they  first  adopted 
these  rites  now  after  several  generations  have  recovered  their  typical 
numbers. 

These  data  on  the  Australian  aborigines  further  suggest  that  an  in- 
dividual can  shift  his  participation  from  one  level  of  social  organization  to 
another,  provided  there  are  cultural  means  for  channeling  such  participa- 
tion. Duff  and  Kew  (1957)  provide  an  account  of  the  recently  extinct 
Kunghit  Haida  Indians  of  British  Columbia,  which  enables  similar  in- 
sights into  basic  group  sizes  in  a  food-gathering  people. 

Their  winter  village  consisted  of  16  to  20  large  houses  (1600  sq.  ft.  of 
floor  space  each).  From  various  of  the  accounts  it  appears  that  the  tribe 
totaled  about  500  individuals,  of  which  slightly  over  200  were  adults.  This 
means  about  10-12  adults  on  the  average  per  house.  Each  house  was  in- 
habited by  a  kinship  group  or  lineage.  During  the  warmer  months  of  the 
year  each  lineage  group  left  the  winter  village  for  its  own  hunting  territory. 
Like  the  Australian  aborigines,  these  British  Columbia  Indians  also  appear 
to  have  a  basic  group  size  not  diverging  far  from  12  and  an  assembly  of 
these  into  a  tribe  of  around  200  adults. 

Incipient  agriculture,  in  which  plow  and  draft  animals  are  absent,  repre- 
sents an  even  more  ad\'anced  efficiency  of  food  extraction,  characterized 
by  a  permanent  village.  The  Jarmo  site  in  Iraq,  inhabited  some  6700  years 
ago,  presumably  represents  a  typical  village  at  this  level  (Braid wood  and 
Reed,  1957).  Braidwood  and  Reed  estimate  that  150  persons  (50  adults) 
inhabited  the  25  houses  located  there.  This  type  of  village  structure  ex- 
tends into  the  present.  The  mean  size  of  185  villages  in  this  part  of  Iracj 
is  140,  which  presumably  represents  46-56  adults. 

From  the  scanty  examination  of  lower-order  basic  group  sizes  in  man 
we  shall  skip  to  the  urban  society  of  a  modern  nation,  the  United  States. 
The  social   organization  represented   by  Australian   aborigines  and  the 


100  John  B.  Calhoun 

Kunghit  Haida  suggests  that  each  larger  semiclosed  social  system  includes 
within  it  all  the  culturally  limited  basic  group  numbers.  But  even  if  some 
are  skipped  or  unrecognizable,  the  one  group  structure  which  must  be 
preserved  is  that  of  12  adults.  Recent  studies  by  Zimmerman  and  Broderick 
(1954)  and  Zimmerman  and  Cervantes  (1960)  confirm  this  suspicion. 
Their  approach  has  been  to  focus  on  any  given  family,  designated  the  ego 
family,  and  then  to  determine  with  how  many  other  families  its  members 
have  frequent  and  close  associations.  These  latter  are  designated  as  friend 
families.  Absence  of  divorce  or  desertion,  juvenile  arrest,  or  children  not 
completing  high  school  comprised  criteria  for  judging  a  family  as  "good" 
or  "successful."  Presence  of  these  traits  were  used  to  delimit  the  "bad"  or 
"unsuccessful"  families.  Values  held  by  a  family  were  judged  on  the  basis 
of  their  religion,  region  of  origin,  income  level,  and  kinship  bonds.  The 
good  ego  families  typically  have  five  friend  families  with  whom  they  have 
a  high  coincidence  of  values,  and  furthermore,  if  the  ego  family  is  char- 
acterized as  good  most  of  the  friend  families  are  likely  also  to  be  so  char- 
acterized. On  the  other  hand,  bad  ego  families  generally  have  fewer  friend 
families  and  they  are  likely  to  differ  from  them  with  respect  to  the  value 
traits.  The  fewer  the  values  shared  by  the  several  families  forming  such  a 
cluster,  the  smaller  the  cluster  will  be  and  the  greater  the  probability  that 
each  family  will  be  characterized  by  one  or  more  of  the  traits  denoting  it 
as  an  unsuccessful  family. 

The  ideal  state  then  appears  to  be  six  families,  12  adults,  composed  of 
an  ego  family  and  five  friend  families.  Shared  values  bind  such  a  cluster 
despite  the  dispersal  of  the  member  families  through  the  local  community. 
Furthermore,  each  friend  family  in  a  particular  cluster  is,  as  an  ego  family, 
the  center  of  another  cluster.  In  this  way  an  extension  of  the  cluster  de- 
velops to  include  26  total  families.  Although  similar  bonds  between  families 
may  include  a  larger  network,  insofar  as  any  particular  family  is  concerned 
the  25  friend  families  and  extended  friend  families  form  the  limit  of  de- 
pendence and  social  support  relationships.  This  approximation  of  50  adults 
of  the  family-friend  cluster  further  argues  for  the  reality  of  Nb  =  50  as  a 
basic  grouping  revealed  also  in  Birdsell's  supra-horde  of  Australian  abo- 
rigines and  of  the  incipient  agricultural  village  of  the  Jarmo  type.  Reduction 
of  the  size  of  the  family  cluster  below  the  optimum  of  six  when  values  held 
by  member  families  diverge  from  each  other  represents  another  example  of 
the  principle  of  group  fragmentation,  enunciated  by  Birdsell,  which  follows 
a  clash  in  values.  Zimmerman  and  Cervantes  refer  to  this  conflict  as  a 
"confusion  of  values." 

All  the  information  in  this  section,  when  viewed  as  a  whole  and  in  the 
context  of  the  earlier  sections  concerning  the  evolution  of  a  basic  group 
size,  suggests  the  following  tentative  generalization:  Modern  man  derived 


1.  The  Social  Use  of  Space  101 

from  his  primate  and  preprimate  ancestors  a  physiology  transpiring  in 
groups  within  the  range  of  10-20  adults.  This  physiology  was  fixed  some 
half  a  million  years  ago  and  has  not  significantly  diverged  from  it  since. 
Development  of  a  larger  social  group  is  made  possible  by  a  culture  in  which 
a  normative  orientation  prescribes  values,  and  sanctions  roles  of  behavior 
such  that  the  total  effect  of  participation  in  a  larger  group  so  buffers  the 
individual  that  at  any  particular  time  the  individual  functions  socially  as  if 
he  were  a  member  of  a  group  of  12  individuals.  Furthermore,  genetic 
changes  of  the  central  nervous  system  making  learned  value  systems  of 
cultures  possible  must  have  arisen  under  circumstances  which  prevented 
division  of  the  basic  group  size  when  it  reached  twice  this  level.  Either  an 
ecological-psychological  trap  like  the  behavioral  sink  (Section  XII,  C)  or 
any  isolated  but  very  abundant  source  of  a  needed  resource  would  be  ade- 
quate to  demand  either  a  genetic  change  of  physiology  making  life  in  large 
groups  tolerable,  or  a  genetic  change  endowing  the  central  nervous  system 
with  the  capacity  to  learn  and  culturally  transmit  values.  Each  increase  in 
group  size  is  associated  with  a  reorientation  of  the  value  system.  Such  in- 
creases in  group  size  are  saltatory.  The  theoretical  basis  of  why  such 
changes  must  be  saltatory  and  not  transitional  is  discussed  in  Section 
XIII,  B,  3. 


XIII.  A  Formulation  of  Group  Dynamics 

Twelve  individuals  represent  the  approximate  optimum  group  size  for 
certain  species  (Sections  VIII,  C  and  XII,  E) .  Furthermore,  such  a  sized 
group  might  be  expected  to  have  evolved  from  home  range  dynamics. 
Evidence  could  readily  be  assembled  that  other  basic  A^'s,  NbS,  characterize 
other  mammahan  species.  Some  typically  live  as  pairs  while  others  assemble 
in  herds  exceeding  100  or  1000  individuals.  However,  circumstances  may 
force  N  to  diverge  markedly  from  A^6.  Elaboration  here  of  the  model  of 
social  interaction  presented  in  pages  349-354  of  Calhoun  (1957)  provides 
insight  into  the  consequences  of  such  divergence  of  A^  from  Nb. 

A.  The  Model  of  Social  Interaction 

On  a  presumptive  basis  there  are  three  variables  which  should  determine 
the  mechanics  of  contact  and  interaction.  These  are  (a)  the  number  of 
animals  moving  about  and  having  opportunity  of  contacting  each  other, 
(b)  the  length  of  the  refractory  period  following  the  response  of  one  animal 
upon  contacting  another  until  it  is  again  capable  of  exhibiting  a  similar 


102  John  B.  Calhoun 

response,  and  (c)  the  amount  of  space  in  which  the  movement  of  A''  in- 
dividuals takes  place.  Random  distribution  of  positions  of  individuals  at 
any  moment  in  time  is  assumed.  We  choose  to  ignore  a  small  correction 
factor  arising  from  the  fact  that  all  individuals  move.  Velocities  of  all  in- 
dividuals are  initially  assumed  to  be  a  constant.  Furthermore,  we  assume 
that  all  individuals  are  identical.  Thus  our  concern  is  not  which  individuals 
meet,  but  rather  which  state,  responsive  or  refractory,  the  contacting  in- 
dividuals happen  to  be  in. 

N  =  Number  of  animals  forming  the  group. 

d  =  The  diameter  of  interaction  for  each  animal,  that  is,  that  distance 
between  the  centers  of  two  individuals  at  which  a  physical  or 
psychological  collision  or  contact  occurs.  In  the  simplest  case 
animals  may  be  considered  equivalent  to  billiard  balls.  Then  d 
represents  the  diameter  of  the  ball,  the  individual.  See  Section 
XIII  A,  1  for  further  elaborations. 

Assume  an  animal  moving  in  some  direction  on  the  plane  in  a  population, 
A^  —  1,  of  other  animals. 

Each  of  these  other  individuals  presents  a  target  of  dimension  d,  normal 
to  the  X  direction.  The  expectation  that  the  incoming  animal  will  make  a 
collision  while  moving  a  distance  A.r  (in  time  t)  is  the  ratio, 

d(N  -  1)A.T 

of  surface  covered  by  the  targets  to  the  total  surface,  where  A  is  the  area 
available  to  the  animals. 

It  should  be  emphasized  that  the  unit  of  time  must  be  sufficiently  large 
so  that  the  number  of  collisions  in  that  time  interval  is  large  enough  to 
justify  using  the  statistical  law  of  large  numbers  in  the  derivation.  For 
similar  reasons,  it  must  be  assumed  that  the  mean  free  path  of  the  in- 
dividuals must  be  large  in  comparison  with  the  target  diameter. 

Since  the  velocity  v  may  be  considered  ec^ual  to  Ax/t,  the  average  num- 
ber of  contacts  w,-,  per  individual  in  time  /.  is 

djN  -  l)vt 

Tie  = (o2j 

A 

For  present  purposes  we  are  concerned  only  with  the  average  ric  in  t  and 
not  in  the  variability  in  contacts  in  t.  The  frequency  of  contacts  by  a  given 
individual  will  be : 

^.  .  -  .  rf(iV-i)_.  (33) 

{  A 


1.  The  Social  Use  of  Space  103 

Since  d,  v,  and  A  will  be  considered  constants  for  this  presentation,  we  set 

(dv/A)  =  n        so  that        /.  =  m(A^  -  1)  ('^^i 

fjL  reflects  the  ease  of  communication  in  the  sense  of  contacts  per  unit  ot 
time.  Basically,  d,  v  and  A  may  be  specified  in  terms  of  linear  unit,  L. 
Therefore, 

dv       L  •  Lt~^        1  ..,_, 

"  =  1^-6^  =  7  ''•^' 

The  symbol  ^  is  here  used  in  the  sense  of  ''dimensionally  equivalent  to." 
So  by  selecting  appropriate  units  of  time,  n  can  be  made  equal  to  1.0.  In 
following  discussions  n  will  be  considered  equal  to  1.0  in  this  sense  whenever 
the  basic  A^,  Nb,  of  a  species  is  in  an  evolutionarily  steady  state. 

We  will  assume  that  the  population  of  individuals  can  be  divided  into 
two  classes:  [Na],  those  individuals  who  are  in  a  responsive  state,  and 
[Np]  those  individuals  who  are  in  a  refractory  state.  We  further  assume 
that  the  individuals  in  [Na]  will  be  rendered  refractory  either  after  a 
contact  with  a  member  of  the  same  class  or  with  a  member  of  [Np].  After 
any  such  contact,  an  individual  will  remain  in  the  refractory  state  for  a 
length  of  time,  a,  the  refractory  period,  and  after  this  time  has  elapsed 
return  to  membership  in  [N^].  It  is  also  assumed  that  any  contact  that 
an  individual  undergoes  while  it  is  in  the  refractory  state  has  no  influence 
on  the  duration  of  its  refractory  period. 

The  duration  of  such  refractory  periods  must  be  a  function  of  the  be- 
havior of  each  member  of  the  contacting  pair  toward  the  other.  It  is  as- 
sumed that  the  critical  aspect  of  this  behavior  is  its  intensity.  At  the  steady 
state  of  an  Nb  we  shall  first  consider  every  individual  to  be  identical  with 
reference  to  the  intensity  of  its  behavior  toward  others. 

Let: 

la  be  the  intensity  of  action  of  any  member  of  [A^„]  toward  every  associ- 
ate it  encounters. 

ip  be  the  intensity  of  action  of  any  member  of  [Np]  toward  every  associ- 
ate it  encounters. 

It  is  further  assumed  that  the  most  likely  way  that  the  duration  of  the 
refractory  period,  a,  becomes  a  function  of  the  behavior  of  two  individuals 
toward  each  other  is  that  it  results  from  the  product  of  the  intensities  of 
their  behaviors.  Furthermore,  there  must  be  some  factor,  which  will  be 
called  B,  which  governs  whether  a  refractory  period  will  result  from  the 
interaction. 


104  John  B.  Calhoun 

Let: 

Ba  =  1.0  be  the  value  of  this  factor  in  all  members  of  [Na]. 
Bp  =  0  he  the  value  of  this  factor  in  all  members  of  l^Np]. 
aaa  represent  the  refractory  period  resulting  in  each  of  two  members  of 

[_Na]  who  meet. 
a,rp  represent  the  refractory  period  resulting  in  each  member  of  [^Na'] 

which  encounters  a  member  of  [A^p]. 
aj,a  represent  the  refractory  period  resulting  in  each  member  of  [iVp] 

which  encounters  a  member  of  [_Na]. 
app  represent  the  refractory  period  resulting  in  each  of  two  members  of 

[_Np^  which  meet. 

It  follows  that: 

iaia  =  oiaaBa  =  idp  =  dapBa,         aud  all  are  real  values  (36) 

and  that: 

^p^a  ^pa-*^p  ^p^p  ^pp^ p  V7  \*J  i  ) 

In  all  following  discussion  Ba  and  Bp  will  be  omitted  in  discussing  a, 
but  every  mention  of  aaa  and  Uap  will  assume  the  action  of  Ba,  and  likewise 
any  mention  of  apa  and  app  will  assume  the  action  of  Bp. 

It  may  be  objected  that  no  distinction  is  being  made  between  the  two 
kinds  of  contacts,  responsive-responsive,  and  responsive-refractory.  It  is 
perfectly  feasible  to  introduce  two  refractory  periods,  aaa  and  aap,  of  differ- 
ent duration  to  answer  this  objection.  At  the  present  juncture  the  experi- 
mental data  are  so  scanty  that  it  does  not  appear  to  be  fruitful  to  introduce 
additional  complexity,  and  we  have  chosen  to  consider  ana  =  cinp  for  the 
workmg  model  insofar  as  duration  is  concerned. 

An  alternative  model  would  have  been  to  choose  aap  =  0,  that  is,  the 
only  contact  inducing  a  refractory  period  being  a  contact  between  two 
individuals  both  of  whom  are  responsive.  Under  such  an  assumption  the 
number  of  contacts  between  responsive  individuals  in  a  unit  interval  of 
time  w^ould  increase  asymptotically  to  the  value  1/a,  whereas,  as  we  shall 
show,  the  model  adopted  provides  that  the  number  of  contacts  between 
responsive  individuals  passes  through  a  maximum  as  A^  increases. 

Given  sufficient  proximity  of  an  individual  in  the  responsive  state  to 
some  other  indi\'iduals  requisite  to  the  usual  elicitation  of  an  interaction 
or  response  to  denote  a  contact,  evidence  from  certain  mammals  suggests 
a  mechanism  capable  of  blocking  a  social  response.  Such  a  mechanism 


1.   The  Social  Use  of  Space  105 

which  defines  the  probability  of  a  contact  being  socially  "perceived''  shall 
be  called  n'. 

ria  ==  number  of  contacts  made  while  the  given  animal  is  in  the  "re- 
sponsive" state. 

fa  =  (ria/t)  is  the  frequency  of  responsive  contacts  when  the  animal  in 
question  is  in  the  "responsive"  state  over  all  time. 

Since  each  contact  between  two  individuals,  at  least  one  of  whom  is  in 
the  responsive  state,  is  followed  by  a  refractory  period  a,  characteristic  of 
each  of  the  responsive  individuals,  and  since  there  are  ?ia  such  contacts  in 
time  t,  then  the  individual  is  in  the  refractory  state  for  a  total  time  ana. 
Clearly,  the  total  time  ta  in  which  the  individual  is  in  a  responsive  state 
is  t  —  an'Ua. 

Since  na  =  tfa  then 

ta    =    t    -    aix'Ua    =    t    -    afx'tfa    =    t{l    -    OCuJa)  (38) 

In  this  sum  of  refractory  intervals,  ta,  contacts  will  be  made  at  frequency 
/c,  but  all  such  contacts  are  made  while  the  animal  is  responsive  so  that 

Ua    =   fda    =   fct{l     —    CXIl'fa)     =    tfa 

or,  since /a  =  ria/t 

fa    =   /.(I     -    V/a)  (39) 

Thus 

1   —  anja 
It  will  also  be  helpful  to  rearrange  Eq.  (39)  to  obtain /„  as  a  function  of /d 
/.  =  /„  +  ccn'fafc  =/«(!+  an%) 

So 

/«  =  ,  /    .,  (41) 

We  may  also  define  the  frequency  of  refractory  contacts  by  each  in- 
dividual over  all  time 

fp    =fc-fa  (42) 

Contacts  between  individuals  will  be  of  three  kinds:  (a)  both  individuals 
responsive;  (b)  both  refractory;  and  (c)  one  individual  responsive  and 
the  other  refractory.  A  given  individual  meets  fc  other  individuals  in  unit 


106  John  B.  Calhoun 

time.  Of  these  J\  contacts,  fa  are  with  individuals  in  a  responsive  state. 
Hence,  the  probabiHty  pa  that  any  given  encounter  will  be  with  a  responsive 
individual  will  be 

Pa    =     (fa/f.)  (43) 

Therefore,  of  all  the  encounters  /„  in  unit  time  which  the  given  individual 
makes  while  it  is  responsive,  the  number 

faa    =    Pa  fa  (44) 

will  be  with  other  responsive  animals.  Thus,  /„<,  may  be  considered  the 
(absolute)  frecjuency  of  responsive-responsive  encounters.  Substituting 
(43)  into  (44)  we  obtain 

faa    =    y  (45) 

Using  Eq.  (41) 

•^'"'  ^  /.(I   +  an%r  ^    (1   +  «M7e)2  ^^^^ 

In  like  manner  we  can  define  fpp  as  the  frequency  of  contacts  of  individuals 
both  of  whom  are  refractory  and  of  fap  for  the  freciuency  of  contacts  in 
which  one  individual  is  responsive  and  the  other  refractory. 

By  an  argument   analogous  to   that   given  above   we   arrive   at   the 
formulation 

fr>.='^  =  ^^^^-^  (47) 

Je  Je 


Using  Eq.  (41) 
J  pp  ~ 


^'  Vl     +    V/JJ/    -^  -^      \         l+V/e         / 


(1  -^an%y 

Again: 


(48) 


/..  =  -^  =  ?^^^I^  (49) 


1.   The  Social  Use  of  Space 

And  using  Eq.    (41) 

Jap 


107 


2/. 


1  +  an% 
2 


1  +  an'fc 

(1  +a/z70- 


•/.(I  +  an%  -   1) 
1  +  an'fc 


f50) 


We  can  obtain  an  explicit  relation  between  /„p  and  N  and  /aa  and  N  by- 
substituting  Eq.  (34)  respectively  into  Eqs.  (50)  and  (46) : 


And 


Jap 


J  aa 


2aix'tx-{N 


1 


[1   +aMM'(iV  -    \)J 

y.{N  -  1) 

[1  +a,x,i'{N  -   \)J 


(51) 


(52) 


The  function  /««  =  0  when  A^  =  1  and  also  faa  approaches  0  as  A"  tends  to 
infinity.  Since  faa  is  continuous  and  differentiable  for  all  positive  values  of 
A",  it  has  a  maximum  A'^,  for  some  value  of  fan  at  which  the  derivative  of 
faa  w'ith  respect  to  A"  is  zero 


dfa 


dNr 


=  0  = 


and 
Hence 


m[1  +  ann.'{N  -  !)][!   -  ann'jN  -  1)] 

[1  +«m/(A^-  1)? 

1   -  auitx'{N„.  -   1)    =  0 


Nm-     \     = 


1 


1 


afiix 


or 


ocuii 


Nm    -    1 


(53) 


(54) 


(55) 


Thus,  the  larger  a,  the  smaller  Nm.  In  other  words,  the  position  of  the 
maximum  shifts  to  the  left  as  a  increases.  In  order  to  find  the  maximal 
value  of  faa,  faV,  we  may  substitute  Eq.  (55)  into  (52)  obtaining 

/i:^  =  1/4V  (56) 

In  other  words,  the  number  of  responsive-responsive  contacts  in  unit 
time  decreases  as  the  refractory  time  increases.  Since  the  refractory  time. 


108  John  B.  Calhoun 

a,  is  assumed  to  increase  when  the  intensity  of  interaction,  i-,  increases, 
faa  decreases  as  intensity  of  interaction  increases. 

When  the  A^"  of  a  species  has  attained  an  evolutionary  steady  state, 
designated  as  Nb,  n  and  ix'  will  each  have  values  of  1.0.  When  n  =  1.0  it 
will  be  designated  tib.  Existence  of  an  A''^  steady  state  does  not  mean  that 
the  temporal  A^  camiot  fluctuate  within  the  lifetime  of  a  species  or  the 
history  of  a  population.  Rather,  it  means  there  is  a  particular  A^  compatible 
with  ixb-  At  this  Nb,  with  its  /X6,  all  contacts  are  perceived.  That  is,  n'  =  1.0, 
and  whenever  /jl'  =  1.0  it  will  be  designated  fxb-  Not  only  will  all  contacts 
by  responsive  individuals  be  perceived,  but  each  member  of  A^6  will  interact 
with  the  same  average  intensity  and,  thus,  a  becomes  ab.  Obviously  Nb 
is  the  Nm  toward  which  a  species  "strives."  In  this  "striving,"  which  may 
be  either  maturational  or  evolutionary  in  terms  of  units  of  time,  A^  may 
vary  as  a  function  of  ex,  or  a  may  vary  as  a  function  of  A''.  At  that  A^  it 
follows  from  Eqs.  (55)  and  (56)  that: 

a5/i:^  =  0.25  (57) 

This  holds  for  all  A^;,. 

<*b/ia^  defines  the  maximum  satisfaction  from  social  interaction  and 
will  hereafter  be  referred  to  as  db. 

The  usual  intensity,  i,  of  interaction,  which  determines  a,  since  P  =  a, 
may  be  considered  as  basically  under  genetic  control.  Similarly,  n  and  m' 
may  be  considered  to  be  normal  expressions  of  genetic  factors  in  so  long 
as  A^6  is  approximately  realized  and  the  members  of  N  experience  conditions 
in  harmony  with  their  genetic  constitution,  that  is  to  say  that  the  environ- 
mental conditions  approximate  those  usually  experienced  by  the  species 
for  many  prior  generations.  However,  abnormal  environmental  circum- 
stances may  so  alter  physiology  and  condition  behavior  that  i,  n,  and  n' 
diverge  from  the  u,  Hb,  and  Mb'  appropriate  to  A^'fe.  In  these  circumstances  n 
and  n'  no  longer  each  equal  1.0,  nor  is  u  in  harmony  with  A''^  in  the  sense 
that  afaa  will  lead  to  maximum  satisfaction  from  social  interaction.  Yet, 
regardless  of  how  i,  fi,  and  fi'  have  diverged  during  maturation,  this  maxi- 
mum may  be  attained  if  the  species  adjusts  by  attaining  that  A^,  different 
from  Nb,  such  that : 

a/i:^  =  0.25  =  d':'  (58) 

Theta,  the  maximal  and  also  optimal  satisfaction  from  social  interaction, 
is  here  designated  as  d^"^  or  just  6o  to  indicate  its  possible  attainment  at 
some  other  N  than  A^6. 

Interactions  whose  frequency  has  been  designated  by  fap  require  special 
consideration. 


1.  The  Social  Use  of  Space  109 

For  clarification  :/ap  =  fap  +  f^a 

And  f^p   =  frequency  with  which  a  given  responsive  individual  interacts 

with  refractory  individuals,  while  it  is  itself  in  the  responsive 

state. 
And  fpa   =  frequency  with  which  a  given  refractory  individual  interacts 

with  responsive  individuals,  while  it  is  itself  in  the  refractory 

state. 

It  can  be  demonstrated  that  f,'^  =  f^^ . 

Therefore 

Zip  =  0.5/„,  (59) 

If  we  let  a„,  represent  that  a  appropriate  to  A^„,  and  /i™\  then  from  Eq. 
(55) 

1 


Similarly,  for  Nb  with  /x  and  /x'  =  1-0 

1 

A^6  -   1 


(60) 


(61) 


If  we  assign  /^"^  as  the  fap  characterizing  Nm  when  /i^^  represents  the 
maximal  value  of /„„  (e.g.,  see  Eq.  (56)),  then  by  utilizing  Eqs.  (51), 
(52),  (55),  and  (60)  and  considering  the  fact  that  /x'  =  1-0,  it  follows  that 

/S^  =  2/i:^  (62) 

And  from  Eq.  (59)  it  follows  that,  when  n  and  m'  each  equals  1.0, 

fT'  =  fa:'  (63) 

For  clarification,  it  is  to  be  noted  that  Eqs.  (62)  and  (63)  refer  to  the  con- 
dition when  fjL  and  m'  remain  unchanged  at  the  fxb  and  (Xb  values  appropriate 
to  Nb,  but  a  adjusts  to  the  existing  A^  according  to  Eq.  (60)  so  that  the 
existing  N  becomes  an  A^^*")  differing  from  Nb  in  most  instances. 
As  already  demonstrated 

eo  =  af'-'  (64) 

where  do  represents  the  maximal,  and  for  this  special  case  also  the  optimal, 
amount  of  time  an  individual  can  remain  in  that  refractory  state  denoting 
satisfaction  from  social   interaction.  At  iV„„  where  Bo  is  realized  a/„'j"'^ 


110  John  B.  Calhoun 

amount  of  time  is  spent  in  frustrating  refractory  periods. 
Here 

e'r'  =  af:i-'  (65) 


And  from  Eqs.  (63),  (64),  and  (65)  it  is  obvious  that 


qCw)     _    n(m) 


(66) 


Since  Nb  is  a  special,  and  the  most  important,  case  of  Nm,  Eq.  (66) 
represents  a  significant  consequence  of  evolution,  as  well  as  adjustment  to 
current  group  size  different  from  Nb.  It  means  that  when  members  of  a 
group  attempt  to  maximize  satisfaction  from  social  interaction,  they  will 
of  necessity  spend  an  equivalent  amount  of  time  experiencing  frustration 
from  social  interaction.  Evolution  having  transpired  in  such  a  system  of 
social  physics,  physiology  must  be  in  harmony  with  this  normal  degree  of 
frustration.  Likewise,  any  marked  decrease  or  increase  of  /„p  from  /^p"'^ 
should  prove  stressful. 

From  Eqs.  (51),  (59),  and  (61),  when  mm'  and  a  remain  appropriate 
to  Nb  but  A''  fluctuates,  it  follows  that 

^      MN  -  ly- 

As  N  approaches  zero,  /„p  approaches  zero.  As  N  approaches  infinity, 
lap  approaches  the  fc  characteristic  of  Nb,  that  is  when/^  =  Nb  —  I  =  l/ab. 
Yet  at  the  same  time  (see  discussion  following  Eq.  (52))  /„„  approaches 
zero  as  N  approaches  infinity  with  reference  to  its  divergence  from  Nb- 
At  N's  much  larger  than  Nb  the  frequency  of  contacts  resulting  in  refrac- 
tory periods  (i.e.,  faa  and  f^p)  comes  to  approximate  the  total  contacts 
transpiring  in  Nb]  however,  practically  all  of  such  contacts  are  of  the  type 
frustrating  to  individuals  having  returned  to  the  responsive  state. 

1.  Terms  and  Equations 

The  following  assembly  of  definitions  will  facilitate  understanding  later 
discussions.  Insofar  as  possible  the  N  animals  in  the  group  will  serve  as  the 
basis  of  the  definitions.  Some  terms  utilized  in  later  sections  will  also  be 
included  here. 

N  =  Total  niunber  of  individuals  in  the  group.  In  the  strictest  sense, 
a  group  is  defined  by  habitation  of  an  exclusive  area  in  which 
each  resident  member  has  a  good  chance  of  contacting  all  others. 
d  =  Target  diameter  of  an  individual.  In  the  simplest  sense,  d 
specifies  the  actual  physical  diameter  with  the  "animal"  having 
no  more  d  properties  than  a  billiard  ball.  Included  under  d  are 


J.   The  Social  Use  of  Space  ^ 

any  characteristics  such  as  bright  color,  vocalizations,  odoi,  or 
upright  posture  which  enhance  the  hkelihood  of  an  individual 
being  perceived  by  its  associate.  Through  evolution  and  matura- 
tion certain  species,  particularly  man,  acquire  the  capacities  to 
utilize    nonphysical    characteristics    to    alter    target    diameter. 
These  nonphysical  characteristics  include  attitudes  and  values 
whose  possession  influences  the  likelihood  of  the  holder  being 
perceived  and  responded  to  by  his  associates. 
V  =  "Velocity"  with  which  an  individual  ''moves"  through  its  en- 
vironment. It  includes  all  properties  which  enhance  the  likeli- 
hood of  one  individual  approaching  its  associates.  Thus,  in  addi- 
tion to  including  actual  velocity,  it  includes  all  sensory  mecha- 
nisms which  extend  the  individual's  perception  of  others  in  any 
direction  along  its  travel  path.  Thus,  where  r  is  the  radius  of 
perception  beyond  the  physical  bounds  of  the  individual,  v  be- 
comes rLt-\  see  Eq.  (35).  Furthermore,  v  =  rU-^  must  actually 
become  more  complex  than  this.  Animals  further  vary  in  the 
number  of  trips  per  unit  time.  See  previous  discussion  in  Sec- 
tions III,  A,  1  and  A,  4  which  deal  with  how  emotionality  alters 
the  frequency  of  trips.  Therefore,  if  we  let: 

D^  =  r  =  radius  of  perception 

^2  =  number  of  trips  per  unit  time;  or  any  time  or  place 
pattern  of  movement  which  alters  probability  of  con- 
tacting others 

v-i  .-^  Lt-^   =   actual  velocity 

Then  biological  velocity,  v,  becomes: 

Note :  Here  the  product  is  used  in  the  sense  of  a  function 
of 
When  V  is  considered  in  later  discussions  it  will  have  all  these 
connotations. 

r  =  radius  of  perception  as  discussed  above. 
A  =  area  inhabited  by  the  N  individuals,  each  of  whom  has 
a  good  opportunity  of  contacting  any  other  member 

oiN. 
n  =  (dv/A)    is    a   communication-enhancing    or    contact- 
producing  factor.  By  considering  v  in  its  simplest  sense, 
n  becomes   (drv/A)   as  soon  as  the   perception  sivath 


112  John  B.  Calhoun 

determined  by  the  individual's  capacity  to  perceive 
beyond  its  own  physical  bounds  comes  into  play. 
n'  =  K  communication-inhibiting  or  contact-blinding  factor. 
It  reflects  a  psychological  property  permitting  the  in- 
dividual to  ignore  a  contact  resulting  from  n.  n'  must 
derive  from  {d'r'v' /A')  factors.  See  Section  XIII,  B,  4 
for  further  treatment  of  n' . 
i  =  Intensity  of  action  of  one  individual  toward  another 

upon  contact. 
a  =  ^■^  the  duration  of  the  refractory  period  following  the 
contact  of  a  responsive  individual  with  some  other 
individual.  Contacts  made  by  an  individual  while  it  is 
in  a  refractory  state  have  no  influence  upon  its  a.  In 
some  way  the  refractory  period  is  a  consequence  of  the 
intensity  of  interaction.  It  is  thus  the  result  of  the 
interplay  between  the  action  of  each  individual  toward 
the  other.  I  have,  therefore,  assumed  that  the  product 
of  these  intensities  of  action  represents  a  first  approxi- 
mation of  a  proportionality  to  the  duration  of  the 
refractory  period. 

faa  =  The  frequency  with  which  one  individual,  while  in  the 
responsive  state,  meets  other  individuals,  who  are  also 
in  the  responsive  state. 

/„p  =  The  frequency  with  which  an  individual,  while  in  the 
responsive  state,  meets  nonresponsive  ones  (i.e.,  those 
in  the  a  refractory  state) . 

aaa  =  Refractory  period  produced  in  each  individual  after 
each  of  the  faa  interactions  in  which  it  is  involved.  «„« 
produces  satisfaction. 

cxap  =  Refractory  period  produced  in  the  responsive  individual 
after  each  f^p  interaction.  a„p  produces  frustration  at 
least  in  the  sense  of  being  a  nonspecific  stressor  of 
physiology. 

ocaa  =  ocap  wlth  regard  to  duration. 
da  or 

0W  =  afaa,  the  amount  of  time  per  unit  time  spent  in  satis- 
fying refractory  periods,  da  represents  the  consequences 
of  positively  affective  interaction. 
Of  or 

^(/)  =  olJ'^j,,  the  amount  of  time  spent  in  the  frustrating  and 
physiologically  stressful  state. 

df  approaches  zero  as  N  approaches  I.O 


1.  The  Social  Use  of  Space  113 

df  approaches  0.25  at  Nb 

df   approaches   1.0   as   A^   approaches   infinity   with 

reference  to  Nb- 
Of  represents  the  consequences  of  negatively  affective 
interactions. 
0<»')  =  amfi":^^  =  maximal  da  =  0.25;  see  Eq.  (57). 
j^(m)  ^  That  A^  at  which  S^"'^  results.  In  other  words,  at  N"" 
satisfaction  from  social  interaction  is  maximized,  but 
at  A'"^'"^  df"^  =  d^"'\  that  is,  there  is  as  much  frustra- 
tion as  satisfaction  from  social  interaction. 
A^6  =  The  basic  group  size  of  a  species  living  under  those 
conditions  to  which  it  is  most  adapted.  A^6  is  a  special 
case  of  N^'"\  0f  \  e^/\  ab,  u,  ixb,  and  Mb'  represent  values 
appropriate  to  Nb-   Here  6^^^  and  df^  always  =  0.25, 
as  may  be  seen  from  Eqs.  (57)  and  (63).  At  Nb  both 
Mb  and  nb    must  equal  1.0.  d^J"\  dj'"^  represent  values 
appropriate  to  A^c-)  in  which  ^i'">   -  ^f  ^  and  0)'">  = 

However,  a^"'^  i^"'\  m^""^  and  m'^'"^  at  A^^'")  may  all 
differ  from  comparable  values  appropriate  to  Nb. 
Q(o)  _  Q{m)  £q^.  ^ij  ^  Q^j^gj.  ^j^g^j^  ^^^  although  quantitatively 

9^^°\  d^J"\  and  da'"^  all  =  0.25. 
No  =  Any  A^  when  da  =  d^°\  No  may  equal  Nb,  but  when  it 
differs  from  Nb,  some  alteration  in  n,  ij.',  or  a  permits 
attainment  of  the  optimum  6 a,  that  is  dj  . 

2.  Interaction  P'unctioxs  Stated  in  Terms  of  A^ 

The  number  of  indi\iduals  inhabiting  an  area  is  more  readily  measured 
than  any  other  function  relating  to  this  model  of  social  interaction.  There- 
fore, it  will  be  helpful  to  state  all  other  functions  in  terms  of  A^: 

/.  =  n{N  -  1)  (34) 

If  intensity  of  interaction  is  labile  to  the  point  that  0^'"*  can  always  be 
attained,  then 

^  (60) 


which  means  that : 


jlmlJ-m   {N m    —     1) 


\n,ny^n^'{Nm    "     1)/ 


(68) 


JJ4  John  B.  Calhoun 

A  core  aspect  of  this  thesis  is  that  whenever  ju  increases  above  m6  there 
will  be  compensatory  shifts  in  m'  such  that  mm'  will  again  eciual  1.0.  There- 
fore, a  and  i  will  gradually  become  a  function  of  N„„  or  we  might  rather 
say  that  the  members  of  the  group  attempt  to  adjust  their  intensities  of 
interaction  to  make  any  existing  A'',  regardless  of  how  much  it  has  diverged 
from  Nb,  become  A„,.  Thus,  Eqs.  (60)  and  (68)  become: 


Oim     — 


N  -  1 


(69) 


(70) 


At  Nb  it  follows  from  Eqs.  (56)  and  (69)  that  the  maximum  frequency 
of  interaction  of  one  responsive  individual  with  other  responsive  ones 
becomes : 

Hrn)     ^    ^f    -     1  (71) 

J  aa  A 

Accepting  the  logic  above  that  in  time  all  individuals  will  attempt  to  ad- 
just their  intensity  of  interaction  compatible  with  any  existing  N,  it 
follows  that: 

A  —  1 

fm)     ^    f! i  (72) 

Jaa  ^  ^         ^ 

It  further  follows  from  Eqs.  (02)  and  (69),  where  mm'  tend  to  adjust  to 
1.0  and  i  adjusts  to  maximize  <?„  regardless  of  change  in  N,  that: 

.(.)   .  ^(^  -  1)  =  ^(^  -  '^  (73) 

•''"'       [1  +  a(N  -  i)y         4 

And  similarly  at/i"'\  it  follows  from  Eqs.  (51),  (59),  and  (69)  that: 

^         a,HN  -  1)^        ^  ,HN  -  1) 
•'"^  [1  +  a(N  -  l)y  4 

Equations  (73)  and  (74)  must  be  kept  in  mind  while  reading  Section 
XIII,  B.  It  has  already  been  pointed  out  in  the  discussion  following  Eq. 
(66)  that  in  the  evolutionary  steady  state  dj"'^  =  di'"\  Here  [see  Eq. 
(63)],  f^a^  =  fai'"\  This  is  a  major  premise  of  this  paper,  that  animals 
"strive"  to  experience  equal  amounts  of  satisfaction  and  frustration  from 
social  interaction.  But  note  what  happens  according  to  Eqs.  (73)  and 
(74)  when  ju  varies.  If  m  increases  and  the  members  attempt  to  optimize 


1.  The  Social  Use  of  Space  115 

satisfaction,  to  attain  6^„"'\  then 

-T-,  =  fi,         or        df  =  ixd^o"'\ 

aim)  ^'  J  r- 

Thus,  excess  frustration  will  increase  proportional  to  the  increase  in  m-  For 
this  reason,  animals  will  always  be  conservative  in  that  they  will  attempt 
to  reject  any  changes  leading  to  an  increase  in  fj.. 

Where  /j.  =  fib  =  1.0  and  A''  becomes  Nm  [also  refer  to  Eq.  (2;"))  ] 

N  -  1 

fim)     ^     jfUn)      ^     1 /75) 

J aa  J  ap  j  \     "/ 

In  some  circumstances  n  may  be  more  labile  than  i  as  an  adjust! ve  mecha- 
nism to  changes  in  A^  from  Nb-  Where  intensity  of  interaction  remains 
constant  at  that  level  appropriate  to  Nb,  a  remains  ab.  And  yet  Eq.  (55) 
reveals  that  the  N  differing  from  A^6  can  become  No  provided: 

No  =  l  +  -^,  (76) 

(XblJio^ 


Also  by  analogy  to  Eq.  (69) : 


ab 


Nb  -  1 
Then  substituting  Eq.  (77)  into  (76) : 


(77) 


iVo  -  1  +  ^^^  (78) 


Therefore : 

,       Nb 


MoM     = 


No  -  1 


(79) 


In  the  original  change  of  No  from  Nb,  m  and  /x'  were  //&  and  nb  and  each 
was  therefore  equal  to  1.0.  However,  we  are  here  concerned  with  the  case 
when  n  is  labile,  that  is,  it  can  become  different  from  /xb.  Furthermore, 
fib  can  be  ignored  since  any  change  in  fib  must  await  some  stability  in  the 
change  of  fx.  Thus,  when  a  remains  at  ab 


,o  =  ^^^  (80) 

^        No-\ 


Where  intensity  of  interaction  remains  constant,  and  N  i  represents  the 


116  John  B.  Calhoun 

value  of  N  at  the  inflection  point  of  /^^,  that  is  at  the  point  where  the 
second  derivative  of  /„<,  as  a  function  of  N  is  zero,  it  may  be  shown  that 

Ni=  \+—  (81) 

abHo 

Then  inserting  Eq.  (77)  into  Eq.  (81) 

;,,.!+  ^J^hjZ^  (82) 

Mo 
Then  inserting  Eq.  (80)  into  Eq.  (82) 

Ni=  \  +  2(No  -  1)  (83) 

Where  A^  is  Ni,  Eq.  (83)  becomes 

iV,  =  1  +  2(iV6  -  1)  -  (84) 

=  2N,-  I  (85) 

B.  Basic  Processes  Involved  in  Social  Interaction 

1.  Satisfaction  and  Frustration  as  a  Function  of  Group  Size 

Satisfaction  and  frustration  from  social  interaction  are  by  definition 
measured,  respectively,  by  da  and  6/.  Full  satiation  attains  at  d^""^  and 
optimum  frustration  at  dj"'\  These  equivalent  cjuantities  are  equally 
necessary  for  the  individual  to  persist  in  an  optimum  state.  We  shall  here 
be  concerned  with  the  effects  upon  6a  and  df  resulting  from  varying  A'' 
when  ab,  fib,  and  fxb  remain  constant  and  appropriate  to  Nb.  It  must  be 
recalled  from  the  statement  preceding  Eq.  (65)  that  df''^  is  not  used  in 
the  sense  of  the  maximum  af'^p  but  rather  as  the  amount  of  time  spent  in 
frustrating  refractory  periods  at  iV„t,  that  N  where  the  maximum  amount 
of  time,  0^J^\  is  spent  in  satisfying  refractory  periods.  As  implied  in  Table 
XI,  B  and  Fig.  35,  af'^p  attains  a  maximal  value  when  A^  =  infinity. 

As  Nb  increases  i  and  a  must  decrease  in  order  to  maintain  d^'"^  [see 
Eqs.  (56),  (61),  and  (69-71)].  Likewise,  for  any  arbitrary  series  of  a, 
such  as  1.0,  0.75,  0.5,  0.25,  0.1,  0.05,  0.025,  and  0.01,  there  must  be  respec- 
tive Nb  at  which  a/^^^  =  0f  \  Each  such  a  with  its  corresponding  Nb 
might  be  considered  as  representing  a  distinct  species.  For  each  species 
circumstances  may  cause  N  to  diverge  from  its  Nb.  In  any  such  divergence 
da  diminishes,  and  for  the  species  where  a  remains  constant,  /„«  will  exhibit 
changes  proportional  to  da.  Thus,  faa  may  be  taken  as  an  index  of  the  degree 
to  which  changes  in  A^  from  Nb  diminish  satisfaction  from  social  interac- 


1.  The  Social  Use  of  Space 


117 


tion.  Such  reductions  of /„„  are  shown  in  Fig.  34;  f^J^\  Eq.  (75),  is  that 
faa  denoted  by  the  point  where  the  dashed  hne  intersects  each  soUd  hne 
curve.  Dropping  vertically  to  the  abscissa  from  each  such  intersection 
defines  the  Nb  for  which  that  a  is  appropriate  in  the  sense  of  optimizing 
satisfaction,  d^J"\  In  each  case,  regardless  of  the  size  of  Nb,  af["^^  =  0.25. 


Fig.  34.  Frequency  of  satisfactory  social  interactions  {faa)  as  a  function  of  density 
(iV)  and  refractory  period  (a) .  The  intersectiori  of  the  dashed  line  and  any  solid  line 
curve  defines  the  basic  group  size,  Nh,  appropriate  to  that  a.  All  «/„„  defined  by  these 
intersections  represent  a  constant,  e^^ ,  which  defines  the  optimum  amount  of  satiation 
which  can  be  experienced  by  any  member  of  an  iVt-  The  values  on  the  ordinate  indicated 
by  N  intersecting  the  dashed  line  represent  /i™ ^  =  (iV  -  l)/4  when  n  and  m'  each 
equals  1.0  or  tin'  =  1.0.  For  any  given /^^\  a  can  be  determined  by  finding  the  A^  at 
which /ao  intersects  the  dashed  line.  Here  a  =  \/{N  —  1). 


Each  A^6,  so  defined,  represents  a  distinct  species  in  the  sense  that  there 
have  arisen  genetic  alterations  in  i,  such  that  there  exists  the  highest 
probability  of  a  steady  state  in  which  «/„„  optimizes  satisfaction  from  social 
interaction. 

Some  of  the  insights  revealed  in  Fig.  34  may  be  more  explicitly  compre- 
hended by  examining  Table  XL  In  the  left-hand  part  A  of  this  table,  sue- 


118  John  B.  Calhoun 

cessive  doublings  of  Nb  are  presented.  For  each  Nb  those  values  for  fc,  u, 
ab,  and  f^a^  are  given  which  are  requisite  for  each  member  to  attain  on  the 
average  an  optimum  satisfaction,  do  =  afaa\  from  social  interaction.  It  is 
apparent  that  as  Nb  increases,  ab  and  ib  decrease,  while  fc  and  f^^^  increase. 
As  the  Nb  group  size  increases,  each  individual  will  have  more  fc  contacts 
with  associates,  of  which  one-fourth  wall  be  satisfying  (i.e.,  fit'a^/fc  =  i). 
For  clarification,  I  might  add  that  for  each  individual  on  the  average  at 
every  Nb,  another  one-fourth  of  the  contacts  are  of  the  frustrating  f'^p 
type,  while  the  remaining  one-half  of  the  contacts  transpire  while  in  the 
refractory  state  which  involve  fp^  or  fpp  contacts.  Values  for  A^6  =  12  are 
shown  in  italics  for  reference  because  of  the  apparent  importance  of  groups 
of  this  size.  It  may  be  seen  that  i  for  Nb  =  12  is  only  one-third  that  for 
Nb  =  2.  Nb  must  increase  from  12  to  121  for  a  similar  decrease  in  intensity 
of  interaction  to  be  necessary. 

In  the  now  voluminous  literature  on  "stress,"  many  papers  deal  with  the 
physiological  repercussions  accompanying  change  in  group  size.  Yet  these 
reveal  little  concerning  how  much  physiological  disturbance  might  be  ex- 
pected to  result  from  a  given  change  in  group  size.  The  prior  model  of 
social  interaction  will  now  be  examined  to  determine  what  insight  the 
model  provides,  under  the  assumption  that  it  approximates  reality. 

Table  XI 
Normative  Relative  Values  of  Interaction  Factors" 


A.  When  N  = 

N, 

B.  When  a  =  0.091 '' 

N 

fc 

a 

i 

Am) 
J  aa 

faa 

0..5fa% 

Oa                   "dd" 

6/ 

2 

1 

1.000 

1.000 

0.25 

0.84 

0.08 

0.07644  0.1736 

0.007 

4 

3 

0.333 

0.577 

0.75 

1.85 

0.51 

0.16835  0.0816 

0.046 

8 

7 

0.143 

0.378 

1.75 

2.62 

1.67 

0.2384     0.0116 

0.142 

12 

11 

0.091 

0.302 

2.75 

2.75 

2.75 

0.250      0.00 

0.25 

16 

15 

0.067 

0.258 

3.75 

2.68 

3.66 

0.2439     0.0061 

0.333 

32 

31 

0.032 

0.180 

7.75 

2.12 

5.99 

0.1929     0.0571 

0.545 

64 

63 

0.016 

0.126 

15.75 

1.39 

7.97 

0.1265     0.1235 

0.725 

128 

127 

0.008 

0.089 

31.75 

0.80 

8.71 

0.0728     0.1772 

0.793 

°  All  values  are  relative  to  the  intensitj'  of  interaction,  1.0,  appropriate  to  a  group  of 
2  individuals,  when  n  =  1.0. 

''  The  a  appropriate  for  Nb  =  12. 

'  0.5  fap  when  a  remains  constant  approaches  1/a  as  N  approaches  infinity.  In  this 
case  with  a  =  0.091,  1/a  =  11.0.  In  other  words  0.5  fap  approaches  iVj  —  1.  0.5  fa,,  is 
used  in  the  sense  of  fap- 


1.  The  Social  Use  of  Space  119 

Two  deviations  from  the  consequences  of  social  interaction  appropriate 
to  Nb  stand  out  as  the  logical  candidates  as  physiological  stressors.  First 
there  is  the  situation  in  which  an  existing  da  is  less  than  the  optimum  ^^"'^ 
"or  do.  This  difference  is  designated  as  the  satiation  deficit,  dd,  where 

0d    =    abfil'^     -    CChfaa    =    do-    da  (86) 

The  second  stressor  is  df.  It  presents  a  philosophical  problem  with  regard 
to  its  assessment  as  a  stressor.  At  A^6,  df  =  do.  The  mechanics  of  interaction 
according  to  the  model  are  such  that  optimizing  (i.e.,  maximizing)  satis- 
faction from  social  interaction  leads  to  an  equivalent  amount  of  frustra- 
tion. During  evofution,  physiology  must  have  been  altered  such  that  it 
became  compatible  with  this  amount  of  frustration.  6;  may  be  thought  of 
as  a  nonspecific  stressor  whose  presence  in  d^"^  amount,  that  is  the  amount 
which  will  arise  when  Nm  is  also  Nh,  is  necessary  for  stimulating  physiology 
to  an  optimum  level.  When  df  is  below  optimum,  df  <  d^/"'\  there  will  be  a 
deficit  in  the  nonspecific  stressors  required  to  maintain  physiology  at 
normal  levels.  Above  optimum  levels,  when  df  >  dj"'\  df  may  be  considered 
truly  as  a  stressor  to  the  extent  that  it  exceeds  d^^K  As  TV  becomes  greater 
than  Nb,  df  becomes  greater  than  the  optimum  value  of  0.25,  and  as  N 
approaches  infinity  df  approaches  1.0.  This  means  that  nonspecific  stressors 
in  the  sense  of  frustration  from  social  interaction  can  never  exceed  four 
times  the  optimum  level.  For  these  reasons  I  choose  to  examine  merely 
how  df  varies  as  a  function  of  A^  rather  than  making  any  effort  to  evaluate 
any  possible  differential  effect  resulting  from  df  being  greater  or  less  than 
the  optimum. 

To  see  directly  how  these  two  stressors,  dd  and  df,  vary  as  N  changes,  a 
specific  case  for  Nb  =  12  is  given  in  Part  B  of  Table  XI.  Here  again,  as  in 
Fig.  34,  it  may  be  seen  that  the  fr.equency  of  satisfactory  interactions, 
faa,  declines  following  either  decreases  or  increases  in  N  from  Nb.  However, 
frustrating  interactions,  f^p  decline  as  A^  declines  below  Nb,  and  likewise 
increase  as  N  increases  above  Nb.  There  results  an  approximately  70% 
deficit  in  satiation  when  N  declines  to  2  or  increases  to  about  122  from  the 
Nb  =  12.  Somewhat  more  marked  changes  from  the  optimum  frustrations 
follow  changes  in  N  from  Nb. 

In  order  that  the  change  in  df  as  a  function  of  the  deviation  of  A^  from 
Nb  may  be  visualized,  they  were  calculated  (Fig.  35)  for  Nb  =  2  and 
Nb  =  12,  for  which  appropriate  intensities  of  interaction  are,  respectively, 
1.0  and  0.302.  Only  increases  in  df  at  N's  above  Nb  are  shown.  As  Nb  in- 
creases from  2  to  12,  the  respective  intervening  curves  for  df  shift  to  the 
right,  that  is,  it  takes  slightly  greater  relative  increases  from  Nb  to  produce 
an  equivalent  increase  in  df.  For  all  practical  purposes  the  df  curves  for 
all  Nb  above  12  are  identical  with  that  of  Nb  =  12.  Initial  increases  in  N 


120 


John  B.  Calhoun 


above  Nb  produce  the  greatest  increase  in  6/.  Later  equivalent  increases  in 
N  produce  less  and  less  increments  to  Of,  frustration. 

Satiation  deficit,  dd,  increases  (Fig.  35)  in  a  somewhat  similar  fashion 
as  df,  but  it  takes  somewhat  larger  increments  in  N  to  produce  comparable 
increments  in  dd.  Whereas  df  most  likely  represents  a  quantity  of  social 
nonspecific  stressors,  Od  most  likely  reflects  emotion  of  a  kind  which  on  the 
human  level  we  call  sadness,  foreboding,  apprehension,  or  home  sickness. 
It  represents  the  physiological  consequences  resulting  from  needed  and 
perhaps  known  social  interaction. 


8=017424 


Fig.  35.  Satiation  deficit  and  frustration  at  greater  than  optimum  group  size. 
The  inset  figure  defines  the  N  of  ma.ximum  decrease  in  the  satiation  d,  per  unit  increase 
oiN. 


A  word  of  explanation  is  in  order  to  reveal  why  I  selected  the  nonfinite 
Nh  =  12.11  instead  of  12.0  for  examining  Od  in  Fig.  35.  In  brief,  I  reasoned 
that  there  must  be  some  Od  not  compatible  with  maintaining  social  life  at 
that  corresponding  A".  When  this  A^  is  reached  there  must  be  some  genetic 
change  transpiring  which  so  reduces  intensity  of  interaction  that  do  may  be 
restored.  On  a  rather  arbitrary  basis,  I  selected  dd  =  0.174,  which  is  equiva- 
lent to  a  70^f  decrease  in  9a  from  do.  Reacquisition  of  do  at  this  N  reciuires 
i  to  decrease  to  0.3  of  its  former  level.  By  such  criteria,  successively  larger 
Ni,  starting  with  Nb  =  2,  will  form  a  series  of  2,  12.11,  124.45,  etc.  Any 


'(3) 


such  saltatorial  series  of  Nb's  may  be  designated  N^  \  Ny,  A" 

]V^"\  The  dd  curves  for  all  A^6  of  12.11  and  above  will  cross  the  0.174 

horizontal  dd  line  within  the  small  black  rectangular  area  superimposed  on 


I.   The  Social  Use  of  Space  121 

this  line  in  Fig.  35.  Further  elaboration  of  such  saltatorial  series  of  Nb  is 
given  in  Section  XIII,  B,  3. 

It  must  be  kept  in  mind  that  a  basic  assumption  underlying  this  general 
formulation  of  social  interaction  is  that  the  mean  free  path  of  an  individual 
must  be  large  in  comparison  with  the  target  diameter  represented  by  any 
other  individual.  That  is,  animals  must  not  be  so  crowded  that  one  in- 
dividual becomes  so  physically  hemmed  in  or  surrounded  by  a  few  others 
that  opportunity  to  contact  many  of  its  associates  becomes  markedly  re- 
duced. When  an  experimental  study  violates  this  condition,  what  has  been 
said  in  the  above  statements  will  not  apply. 

2.  The  Buddixg  Off  of  Social  Groups 

As  the  group  size  increases  beyond  Nb  both  the  deficit  in  satiation,  dd, 
and  the  amount  of  frustration,  9f,  increase  in  so  long  as  intensity  of  inter- 
action remains  constant.  Members  of  the  group  will  find  participation  in 
it  both  less  and  less  satisfactory  and  more  and  more  stressful.  The  question 
arises,  "At  what  point  will  members  find  conditions  so  unbearable  that 
they  will  leave  or  at  which  the  group  will  split?"  Changes  in  da,  or  da,  as  a 
consequence  of  changes  in  A^,  are  proportional  to  /„„.  Therefore,  we  would 
like  to  know  if  there  is  some  N  from  which  any  given  change  in  N  brings 
about  a  greater  change  in  faa  than  a  similar  change  from  any  other  N.  The 
second  derivative,  f^'a  =  0,  occurs  when:  A^  =  1  +  2/(a6Mo)  [see  Eq. 
(81)  ].  This  N  shall  be  referred  to  as  Ni. 

For  Nb  =  12.11  this  arithmetic  inflection  point  comes  &t  N  =  23.22. 
As  may  be  seen  from  the  inset  graph  in  Fig.  35  this  N  marks  the  point  of 
maximal  change  in  da  with  a  given  change  in  N.  At  this  point  an  increase 
in  N  produces  a  greater  decrease  in  da  than  a  similar  change  at  any  other 
N  greater  than  Nb.  Furthermore,  at  Ni,  dd,  as  calculated  by  Eq.  (86),  for 
any  N  will  always  be  0.0278,  which  represents  an  11%  deficit  in  do. 

For  howler  monkeys  and  man,  where  the  basic  A''  appears  to  be  about  12 
adults,  the  social  group  size  rarely  exceeds  2Nb  unless,  as  in  the  case  with 
man,  the  next  well-defined  larger  group  is  much  larger.  It  is  for  this  reason 
that  I  suspect  that  N  will  split  or  bud  off  another  group  when  N  approaches 
(2Nb  —  1) ,  the  point  of  maximal  rate  of  change  in  satiation  deficit  as  given 
by  Eq.  (85).  In  essence,  this  line  of  reasoning  says  that  by  the  time  a 
group  nearly  doubles  in  size  from  its  basic  A^,  its  members  will  begin  to  feel 
uncomfortable  in  the  sense  that  they  do  not  find  participation  in  the  group 
sufficiently  satisfactory.  This  will  lead  to  enough  members  leaving  the 
former  group  so  that  within  each  of  the  two  new  groups  interaction  will 
produce  near  optimal  results. 


122  John  B.  Calhoun 

3.  Saltatorial  Chaxges  in  the  Basic  Group  Size 

Within  most  orders,  and  many  lesser  taxonomic  categories,  related 
species  may  be  found  between  which  there  exist  marked  differences  in  the 
typical  group  size.  Caribou  and  elk  characteristically  maintain  large 
herds  in  contrast  to  the  small  groups  or  even  isolated  pattern  of  living  by 
mule  deer  or  moose.  During  the  active  breeding  season,  bats  of  the  species 
Myotis  lucifugus  and  M.  yumanensis  roost  singly  or  in  small  clusters, 
whereas  M.  grisescens  and  il/.  velifer  maintain  large  assemblies  even  during 
the  breeding  season.  Woodchucks,  Marmoia  monax,  tend  to  live  in  isola- 
tion, whereas  black-tailed  prairie  dogs,  Cynomys  luchviciamis,  live  in  large 
colonies. 

Obviously  these  represent  a  select  group  of  comparisons.  Although  I 
shall  not  attempt  to  substantiate  here  the  typical  group  sizes  found  within 
any  fairly  closely  related  series  of  species,  examination  of  many  series 
suggests  that  there  are  within  each  series  several  discrete  basic  group  sizes 
with  an  extensive  range  between  any  two  nearest  sizes  not  represented  by 
any  species.  For  the  present  purpose,  this  conclusion  will  be  accepted  as 
approximating  reality.  Then  the  question  follows:  "What  characteristics 
of  physiology  and  group  interactions  might  lead  to  saltatorial  steps  in 
group  size  which  become  fixed  by  natural  selection  or  cultural  evolution?" 

In  the  first  place,  there  must  be  some  condition  which  induces  animals 
to  assemble  in  far  greater  group  sizes  than  their  Nb,  and  this  condition  must 
remain  sufficiently  strong  to  prevent  splitting  of  the  group  as  it  approaches 
2Nb  —  1  [see  Eq.  (85)].  Any  spatially  restricted  but  locally  abundant 
resource  might  well  so  act,  particularly  if  response  at  the  source  favored 
the  establishment  of  a  behavioral  sink  as  described  in  Section  XII,  C.  In- 
crease in  group  size  beyond  2A^6  —  1  would  accentuate  social  discomfort 
and  stress  in  the  sense  of  increasing  6d  and  6/  (Fig.  3o) .  At  some  point  these 
factors  must  become  so  intense  as  to  produce  sufficient  decrements  in  re- 
production and  survival  to  threaten  the  survival  of  the  species.  There  is 
no  a  priori  basis  for  judging  what  this  threshold  might  be.  Beyond  2A^6  —  1 
each  increment  in  N  produces  a  smaller  increment  of  9d  and  df.  Examination 
of  the  curves  in  Fig.  35  reveals  that  when  dd  is  about  0.70  of  its  maximum 
and  df  is  slightly  over  twice  its  optimum  level,  any  further  increments  to 
N  produce  little  further  change  in  dd  and  6/.  The  horizontal  line  through  all 
curves  defines  this  point  on  each  curve  and  shall  be  considered  empirically 
as  a  limit  beyond  which  further  increases  in  A'^  cannot  be  tolerated. 

If  Ni'^  =  12,  the  limit  is  Ni^^  =  82  for  Of  and  A^f  ^  =  123  for  dd.  It  will 
be  recalled  from  Section  XII,  A  that  Nb  for  the  Norway  rat  appeared  to 
be  about  12.  Although  local  colonies  approximated  this  number,  all  mem- 
bers of  all  colonies  were  forced  to  interact  at  the  single  source  of  food  and 


1.   The  Social  Use  of  Space  123 

• 

water.  Furthermore,  the  entire  population  in  the  quarter-acre  pen  sur- 
rounded by  a  rat-proof  fence  made  a  closed  system  out  of  the  entire  popu- 
lation. At  123  adults  (Table  X),  marked  disturbance  was  in  evidence, 
witli  only  a  minority  of  the  females  reproducing  successfully.  In  the  closed 
systems  contained  within  a  smaller  area,  described  in  Section  XII,  C, 
severe  reproductive  disturbance  characterized  an  A''  of  slightly  less  than  80 
adults.  Therefore,  for  the  Norway  rat  at  least,  when  the  actual  A''  ap- 
proaches the  increase  above  Nb  presumed  to  represent  the  tolerance  limit, 
a  degree  of  physiological  disturbance  of  sufficient  magnitude  arise  as  to 
indicate  a  necessity  for  some  evolutionary  adaptation  to  the  increase  in 
group  size  for  continued  survival.  Parenthetically,  I  might  add  that  this 
tolerance  limit  for  dd  and  6/  was  arbitrarily  assigned  simply  with  reference 
to  the  slope  of  the  curve,  and  without  prior  knowledge  that  this  level 
would  lead  to  tolerance  limit  A^'s  so  closely  approximating  those  observed 
in  my  experimental  studies. 

One  type  of  evolutionary  change  which  will  reinstate  6  to  its  optimum 
level,  that  will  eliminate  dd,  involves  reducing  intensity,  i,  of  interaction. 
It  will  be  recalled  that  i-  =  a,  the  refractory  period  following  interaction. 
Now  if  Nb  =  12,  and  the  tolerance  limit  A^  is  123,  a  reduction  of  a  from 
0.091,  the  a  appropriate  to  Nb  =  12,  to  0.0082,  the  a  appropriate  to  Nb  = 
123,  will  return  d  to  its  optimum  value  of  0.25.  The  change  in  i  is  0.3  of  its 
level  at  the  former  A^'b,  that  is,  from  i  =  0.3  to  i  =  0.09. 

With  a  hereditary  change  in  behavior  amounting  to  a  reduction  of  i  to 
0.3  its  former  level,  A^6  changes  from  12  to  123.  These  would  then  represent 
two  species,  the  stem  one  having  an  A^^^^  =  12  and  residing  under  those 
environmental  conditions  not  necessitating  an  evolutionary  change  in 
physiology  and  behavior,  and  the  derived  one  having  an  A^^^^  =  123  and 
residing  in  the  presence  of  those  environmental  conditions  forcing  the 
maintenance  of  group  size  far  above  that  of  the  stem  species.  If  this  process 
is  repeated  each  time  9d  reaches  a  tolerance  limit  of  approximately  0.607  of 
the  minimum  dd  possible,  there  arises  what  might  be  termed  a  "satiation 
deficit  saltatorial  series  of  basic  N's"  which  are  as  follows,  starting  with 
A^,^^^  =  12. 


Ni' 

12 

Ni' 

123 

Ni' 

1,359 

Ni' 

15,088 

Ni' 

167,645 

Ni' 

1,862,544 

Ni' 

20,695,365 

A^r 

229,937,917 

124  John  B.  Calhoun 

Each  successive  Nh  will  have  an  intensity  of  interaction  approximating 
0.3  that  of  the  preceding.  At  the  8th  and  last  listed  Nb,  intensity  of  inter- 
action would  be  only  0.0002  that  when  Nh  =  12.  It  seems  rather  patent 
that  no  meaningful  behavior  could  transpire  with  such  a  reduced  intensity 
(duration)  of  interaction.  Two-hundred  thirty  million  adults  in  a  semi- 
closed  social  system  can  only  apply  to  the  world  as  a  whole  for  the  human 
species.  Reduction  of  intensity  of  activity  as  a  means  of  recovering  satia- 
tion from  social  interaction  could,  in  evolutionary  terms,  likely  suffice  in 
mammals  to  the  third  stage  of  15,000  adults  which  entails  a  reduction  of  i 
to  0.09  of  that  appropriate  to  A''^  =  12. 

A  similar  series  of  Nb  can  be  calculated  with  reference  to  Of  =  0.775  or 
the  tolerance  limit  involved  in  shifting  Nb  from  12  to  82.  Optimum  df  = 
0.25  can  be  regained  if  at  this  limit  i  is  reduced  to  about  0.36  of  its  intensity 
at  the  former  Nb.  Such  a  "frustration  saltatorial  Nb  series"  becomes: 


Nt'^ 

12 

Ni'^ 

82 

^(3) 

597 

m^^ 

4,491 

^(5) 

32,343 

Nr 

238,153 

iV^' 

1,753,772 

^(8) 

12,914,892 

Again  this  series  becomes  rather  absurd  at  the  upper  limit  because  of 
the  great  demand  for  reducing  intensity  of  interaction.  Since  semiclosed 
systems,  at  least  on  the  human  level,  and  occasionally  with  other  mammals 
do  approach  some  of  these  A^6,  we  must  ask  what  other  avenues  of  evolu- 
tion exist. 

For  this  we  must  assume  that  intensity  of  interaction  remains  constant 
at  some  level  approximating  that  for  Nb  =  12,  but  that  a  tolerance  limit 
for  dd  and  df  exists.  At  the  A^  of  these  limits  a  change  in  behavior  may  take 
place  which  insulates  the  individual  by  producing  subaggregates  in  which, 
for  all  practical  purposes,  the  individual  at  any  particular  time  is  a  mem- 
ber of  a  subgroup  in  which  Nb  =  12,  even  though  many  other  subgroups 
exist  in  the  environs.  The  individual  may  be  a  member  of  several  such 
groups  but  participates  in  only  one  at  a  time.  Such  changes  in  behavior 
can  be  considered  to  be  of  either  genetic  or  cultural  origin.  In  either  case, 
so  long  as  any  tolerance  limit  for  6/  and/or  6d  exists,  there  must  be  salta- 
torial steps  between  successive  Nb,  and  only  a  few  such  steps  are  possible 
even  if  the  tolerance  limit  arises  at  a  somewhat  lower  level  than  hypothe- 
sized above. 

If   later   research   supports   this   hypothesis   it   will   haxe   considerable 


7.   The  Social  Use  of  Space  125 

bearing  on  our  understanding  of  the  course  of  evolution  involving  change 
in  group  size.  It  will  mean  that  gradual  changes  in  heredity  or  culture  will 
rarely  have  transpired.  Rather,  from  the  pool  of  gene  variability  accumu- 
lated in  the  species,  there  will  be  rapid  shifts  in  gene  frequencies  of  many 
genes,  thus  resulting  in  a  new  phenotype.  In  so  long  as  environmental 
conditions  facilitate  maintenance  of  its  Nb  by  a  species,  its  gene  pool  may 
become  quite  diverse  through  the  accumulation  of  mutant  genes.  Then, 
once  environmental  circumstances  force  the  species  to  maintain  an  ele- 
vated A''  near  its  tolerance  level  for  dd  or  9/,  an  extreme  selection  pressure 
will  arise  for  reducing  the  frequency  of  all  genes  except  those  which  adapt 
the  species  to  its  new  N.  A  genetically  variable  A^^^^  species  will  thus 
rapidly  be  transformed  into  a  genetically  rigid  Nl"^^  species. 

On  the  cultural  level  such  a  process  of  saltatorial  change  in  basic  group 
size  demands  that  the  value  system  which  dictates  acceptable  roles  of 
action  and  communication  be  preserved  even  after  the  usual  group  size 
has  far  exceeded  the  Nb  appropriate  for  that  value  system.  At  the  same 
time,  under  the  pressure  of  increases  in  6d  and  6/,  small  segments  of  the 
closed  system  will  develop  values  divergent  from  the  main  group.  At  the 
tolerance  limit  of  dd  and  6/,  when  A^  has  so  diverged  from  Nl^\  there  will 
arise  a  marked  and  rapid  shift  to  the  prevalence  of  those  newer  values 
appropriate  to  d^'"^  and  9f"'^  at  N^'^K  Value  frequencies  and  gene  frequencies 
become  isomorphic  in  these  two  avenues  through  which  there  can  be  a 
saltatorial  evolution  from  one  basic  group  size  to  another. 

Basic  group  size  for  adults  only  in  the  primary  steps  of  human  cultural 
evolution  seem  to  include  the  10-16  range,  50,  200,  and  2,000.  This  series 
resembles  neither  of  the  hypothetical  saltatorial  group  size  series  except  in 
its  saltatorial  character.  The  hypothetical  series  merely  demonstrated  the 
kind  of  changes  following  from  stated  assumptions.  The  exact  series 
followed  by  any  line  of  change  depends  upon  the  threshold  tolerance  limit 
for  dd  and  6/  as  well  as  three  factors  ignored  in  our  discussion  up  to  the 
present.  Discussion  up  to  this  point  assumes  /x  =  (dv/A)  =  1.0,  where 
d  represented  the  target  diameter  of  other  individuals,  A  the  area  inhabited 
by  the  N  individuals,  and  v  the  velocity  of  movement  of  individuals.  In 
essence,  m  represented  the  likelihood  in  time  t  of  one  individual  encounter- 
ing another. 

It  can  readily  be  shown  from  Eqs.  (52)  and  (60)  that  da,  the  satiation 
from  social  interaction,  i.e.,  afaa,  can  remain  constant  regardless  of  changes 
in  fjL.  At  least  this  is  so  if  the  physiology  and  behavior  of  the  species  is  com- 
pletely adjustive.  From  the  general  form  of  Eq.  (60)  where  n'  =  1.0, 
a  =  1/[m(A^  —  1 )  ],  it  follows  that  each  doubling  of  /x,  that  is  doubling  the 
likelihood  of  one  indi\'idual  meeting  another,  necessitates  a  halving  of 
a,  and  thus  reduces  intensity  of  interaction  from  (a)^'^  to  (a)^/-/2.  If  we 


126  John  B.  Calhoun 

follow  the  prior  assumption  that  each  species  has  an  optimum  intensity  of 
interaction,  then  each  increase  in  n  will  have  an  analogous  effect  to  in- 
creasing group  size.  In  other  words,  increasing  n  above  1.0  will  increase 
dd  and  df.  When  we  are  concerned  with  the  effects  of  changes  in  m  but  as- 
sume a  remains  static  at  the  value  appropriate  to  m  =  1-0,  then  a  must  be 
calculated  from  Eci.  (60)  with  /x'  =  1-0  and  faa  calculated  with  this  a  by 
using  Eq.  (52)  above  and  some  value  of  n  different  from  (Xb  =  1.0. 

For  example,  consider  Nb  =  12.  Then  ab  =  0.091,  and  db  =  0.25  (see 
Table  XI).  If  A''  doubles  and  /x  remains  1.0,  d„  becomes  0.219,  but  if  A^ 
remains  constant  at  Nb  but  m  doubles  to  2.0,  6  becomes  0.195.  Thus,  a 
comparable  increase  in  ^  produces  a  greater  deficit  in  satiation,  6^,  from 
social  interaction,  than  does  a  double  of  A^. 

Thus,  saltatorial  evolution  of  A^^^^^  to  A^^"^  may  be  necessitated  either 
by  an  increase  in  A^  or  an  increase  in  m-  The  rate  of  change  in  N  and  ijl 
may  well  offset  the  tolerance  limit  of  da  or  6/  and  thus  affect  the  magnitude 
of  the  shift  from  A''^^^  to  N^^^K  dv  essentially  measures  the  rate  of  com- 
munication and  A  the  space  within  which  this  communication  takes  place. 
Thus,  n  will  increase  if  A  remains  constant  and  dv  increases,  or  if  dv  re- 
mains constant  and  A  decreases.  If  both  the  rate,  that  is  means,  of  com- 
munication increases  and  the  distance  over  which  communication  must 
take  place  decreases,  m  will  increase  very  rapidly.  Detailed  consideration  of 
communication  is  given  in  the  following  section. 

4.  The  m  Communication  Function 

We  have  already  seen  that  n  =  (dv/A),  as  defined  by  the  previous 
Eqs.  (35)  and  (80),  is  a  communication-enhancing  or  contact-producing 
factor.  (See  prior  discussion  under  Terms  and  Equations,  Section  XIII, 
A,  1.)  Other  than  for  pointing  out  in  the  latter  part  of  Section  XIII,  B,  3 
that  altering  /x  has  much  the  same  consequences  as  altering  N,  we  have 
been  content  to  consider  consequences  of  variability  in  other  functions 
when  IX  remains  constant  at  that  value  /X6  =  1.0  appropriate  to  A^6. 

I  was  led  to  examine  the  question  of  the  consequences  of  varying  /x 
as  a  result  of  the  observation  by  Birdsell  and  by  Zimmerman  and  Cervantes, 
cited  in  Section  XII,  E.  They  observed  that  where  a  conflict  of  values 
arises  in  a  group  there  results  a  reduction  in  group  size.  Here,  we  are  con- 
cerned with  the  special  case  where  attitudes  or  values  comprise  a  major 
aspect  of  the  target  diameter  d.  Each  member  of  the  group  holds  some  n 
number  of  values  by  which  others  recognize  it  as  an  appropriate  object 
for  interacting.  When  some  particular  ^-aIue  is  shared  by  all  members,  it 
may  be  said  to  possess  a  unitary  value  in  contributing  to  target  diameter. 
In  other  words,  under  this  circumstance  all  individuals  possess  the  same 


7.   Tlw  Social  Use  of  Space  127 

target  diameter,  d  =  1.0.  With  reference  to  Eq.  (:i5),  a  unity  value  for 
target  diameter  merely  means  that  there  has  been  genetic  or  cultural 
adaptation  to  the  actual  magnitude  of  d,  such  that  ju  =  dv/A  =  1.0.  How- 
ever, if  an  individual  expresses  a  value  shared  by  only  a  few  of  his  associ- 
ates, he  will  by  this  fact  be  much  more  likely  to  be  perceived  by  his  as- 
sociates, and  thus  more  likely  to  be  reacted  to  by  them.  His  target  diam- 
eter will  be  increased.  Furthermore,  it  is  logical  to  assume  that  the  larger 
an  individual's  target  diameter,  the  greater  will  be  the  response  evoked 
from  associates.  In  so  long  as  all  other  d  xqXwq  characteristics  remain 
identical  among  the  members  of  the  group,  the  one  which  does  vary  among 
members  will  assume  the  sole  role  of  influencing  target  diameter.  As  a 
first  approximation  this  response-evoking  capacity,  which  I  will  call  S, 
of  a  particular  d  value  can  be  taken  as  being  inversely  proportional  to  the 
probability  j)  of  its  being  encountered  among  the  members  of  the  group  of 
N  individuals. 
Therefore : 

S  =  l/v  (87) 

Where  only  one  component  of  d  varies,  and  since  those  shared  compo- 
nents of  d  may  be  ignored,  d  ^  S.  For  the  special  case  where  all  members 
have  the  same  d: 

M  =  {dv)/A  =  {Sv)/A  =  1.0  (88) 

Furthermore,  where  area.  A,  remains  unchanged  at  the  value  appropri- 
ate to  Nb  it  has  the  relative  value  of  1.0.  Therefore: 

Sv  =  1.0 

S  =  l/v  (89) 

And  considering  Eqs.  (87)  and  (89) 

V  ^  p  (90) 

Lastly: 

V  =  l/S  (91) 

A'ariability  of  the  target  diameter  d  among  individuals  means  that  d 
comprises  an  assembly  of  traits,  physical  size,  color,  vocalizations,  behavior, 
and  attitudes  or  values,  d  is  the  total  complex.  Components  shared  by  all 
members  will  be  referred  to  as  {d) .  Those  remaining  traits,  through  which 
an  individual  differs  from  its  associates,  represent  a  genetically  and  cul- 
turally determined  phenotype  to  which  the  response  evoked  from  associ- 
ates is  a  function.  By  response  I  here  refer  solely  to  actions  reflecting  the 
choosing  or  rejecting  of  an  associate.  Such  a  response  may  be  a  function 


128  John  B.  Calhoun 

of  the  trait  itself  or  may  be  a  function  of  a  recognizable  degree  of  differ- 
ence between  one  individual  and  its  most  similar  associate.  1  believe  that 
such  degrees  of  difference  form  the  primary  basis  for  the  maturation  of 
social  behavior  and  social  organization  within  a  group.  Further  treatment 
of  this  topic  follows  in  Sections  XIII,  B,  5,  a  and  b;  XIV,  A  and  B. 

Such  traits  or  degrees  of  difference  comprise  the  units  influencing  social 
behavior.  These  units  will  here  be  called  rf-genes.  As  stated  above  they  may 
be  of  either  hereditary,  or  cultural  origin.  Any  rf-gene,  g^^\  may  develop 
an  allelic  series  of  differing  or  "mutant"  forms  ^i^'  •  •  •  gl^K  When  degrees 
of  difference,  and  not  the  absolute  amount  or  kind  of  difference,  forms  a 
(/-gene  there  can  only  be  two  forms  of  a  particular  t/-gene,  g]l^  and  g^^\ 
where  g\]^^  represents  a  degree  of  difference  from  the  ideal  type,  the 
ideal  d,  and  g^2^  represents  the  retention  of  the  ideal  traits  for  which  g'^^^ 
represents  the  divergence.  c?-genes  of  the  type  g*"^  will  be  called  dominant 
c?-genes,  while  those  of  the  type  g]p  will  be  called  recessive  c?-genes.  d- 
genes  of  the  latter  type  are  treated  in  detail  in  Sections  XIII,  B,  5,  a  and 
b.  Without  specifying  the  allelic  nature  of  any  rf-gene  it  is  obvious  that  the 
target  diameter  d  is  a  function  of  (rf),  g^^\  g^-\  •  •  •,  ^^"\ 

Let 

Si^  represent  the  response-evoking  capacity  of  any  ith.  individual  with 
reference  to  the  probability  of  its  being  chosen  by  associates  as 
an  object  of  affection.  S'"^^  is  related  to  Schaeffer's  love-acceptance 
referred  to  in  Section  XIY,  C. 

aS,^"^  represent  the  response-evoking  capacity  of  any  zth  individual 
affecting  the  probability  of  his  being  rejected  by  associates.  *S^"^ 
is  related  to  Schaeffer's  hostility-rejection  referred  to  in  Section 
XIV  C. 

<S^^^  is  a  function  of  both  (d)  and  the  assembly  of  dominant  d-genes, 
while  <S^"^  is  solely  a  function  of  recessive  c?-genes.  The  probability  of  en- 
countering the  common  (d)  assembly  of  traits  will  be  1.0.  Therefore,  from 
Eq.  (87)  the  positively  affective  stimulus-evoking  capacity  of  this  com- 
monly held  assembly  of  traits  will  contribute  to  the  *S^-^^  of  an  individual 
inversely  proportional  to  the  probability  of  its  being  encountered  within 
the  V  individuals  forming  the  group.  Thus  where  pg  represents  the  prob- 
ability of  encountering  a  particular  c?-gene  and  A,  B,  C,  •  •  • ,  represents  the 
dominant  "allele"  of  c?-genes  (1),  (2),  (3),  •••,  and  there  are  N  —  1 
d-genes  of  the  degrees  of  difference  type,  then: 

S\^'  =—  + -77  +  47+  •••  +-7^17  (92) 

P(d)  Pa  PoB  Pff(Ar_i) 

Similarly  where  a,  b,  c,  •  • ',  N  —  1  represent  recessive  d-genes  of  the 


1.  The  Social  Use  of  Space  129 

degree  of  difference  type: 

Pga  Puh  Pg(.N-i) 

Equation  (92)  applies  strictly  only  for  that  single  individual  which  has 
preserved  all  the  dominant  or  ideal  traits.  For  every  other  individual  one 
or  more  terms  in  Eq.  (92)  will  be  missing,  depending  upon  replacement  of 
the  dominant  d-genes  by  recessive  ones.  Similarly  Eq.  (93)  applies  strictly 
only  to  that  individual  in  which  all  traits,  other  than  those  commonly 
held  (d) ,  have  diverged  from  the  ideal,  that  is  in  that  individual  in  which 
all  non-(d)  rf-genes  are  recessive.  For  all  other  individuals  one  or  more 
terms  in  Eq.  (93)  will  be  missing. 

Consider  the  case  where  all  d-genes  in  a  group  had  been  identical  up 
until  a  particular  point  in  time,  at  which  a  particular  d-gene,  g^^\  "mutated" 
to  g[^'^  in  half  the  members.  In  this  mutation  gf^^'  diverged  sufficiently 
from  g[^^  to  make  quite  distinct  the  derived  from  the  original.  Then  the 
probability  of  each  in  the  group  will  be  only  0.5  and  thus  the  response- 
evoking  capacity  of  each  will  rise  to  a  relative  value  of  2.0.  For  the  average 
individual  S  will  have  increased  from  1.0  to  2.0. 

The  total  Nh  members  of  a  basic  sized  group  will  consist  of  Ni  type  1 
individuals  possessing  g[^^  and  A^2  type  2  individuals  possessing  g[^\  Thus 
ATj,  =  ATi  +  Ni.  Any  individual  will  be  considered  as  being  able  to  encounter 
itself  in  the  sense  of  being  aware  of  its  own  characteristics. 

Considering  this  premise  it  follows  that  the  probability  pi  of  any  type  1 
individual  being  met  by  associates  becomes: 

p,  =  N,/N,  (94) 

Similarly,  the  probability,  pa,  of  type  2  individuals  being  met  by  as- 
sociates becomes: 

p,  =  N2/N,  (95) 

From  Eq.  (87)  it  follows  that  response-evoking  capacity,  *Si,  of  any 
type  one  individual,  and  S2  of  any  type  two  individual  will  be  respectively: 

S,  =  Nb/Ni  (96) 

S,  =  Nb/N2  (97) 

Therefore,  the  mean  response-evoking  capacity,  S,  of  the  Nb  individuals 
becomes: 

Ni-^  N, 


130  John  B.  Calhoun 

Substituting  E(|8.   (9()j  and  (97)  with  (98) 

S  =  2.0  (99) 

The  S  of  Eqs.  (96)  to  (99)  is  essentially  that  of  Eq.  (93)  in  which  g[^^ 
and  ^72^'  become,  respectively,  recessive  d-genes,  g^^^  and  gl^K 

As  may  be  seen  from  Eci.  (98),  whenever  A^i  or  A^2  is  zero,  that  is,  all 
members  of  Nb  have  the  same  target  diameter,  the  response-evoking 
capacity  of  each  member  of  the  group  has  a  relative  value  of  1.0.  However, 
Eq.  (99)  shows  that  as  soon  as  Nb  becomes  divided  into  subgroups  A^i  and 
A''2,  even  though  the  divergent  A''2  has  only  one  member,  the  average 
response-evoking  capacity  doubles.  The  probable  consequence  of  this 
doubling  depends  upon  the  relative  numbers  of  A'":  and  A^2-   Consider 

Table  XII 

The  Influence  of  Relative  Size  of  Subgroups  of  Nb   =   12 
ON  Response-Evoking  Capacity 


Ni 

N2 

s, 

«•: 

S 

12 

0 

1 

0 

1.0 

11 

1 

1.0909 

12 

2.0 

10 

2 

1.2 

6 

2.0 

9 

3 

1.3333 

4 

2.0 

8 

4 

1.5 

3 

2.0 

7 

5 

1.7143 

2.4 

2.0 

6 

6 

2.0 

2.0 

2.0 

Nb  =   12,  then  when  A''!  and  A''2  have  the  sizes  given  below,  members  of 
each  will  have  Si  and  S2  as  shown  in  Table  XII. 

The  jS  of  the  members  of  larger  subgroups  can  never  exceed  twice  the 
optimum  level,  but  the  S  of  the  members  of  smaller  subgroups  has  a 
maximum  of  N  times  that  where  all  members  of  Nb  have  the  same  target 
diameter.  To  understand  the  consequences  to  an  individual  resulting  from 
possession  of  a  large  S,  we  must  inquire  further  as  to  its  implication.  In 
the  first  place,  it  may  evoke  more  frequent  responses  from  associates.  If 
the  group  is  essentially  an  Nb  one,  such  an  individual  will  experience  more 
contacts  than  otherwise  would  be  anticipated.  This  will  have  the  same 
deleterious  consequences  to  him  of  being  in  too  large  a  group.  S  in  this 
case  may  be  thought  of  as  increased  target  diameter,  d.  On  the  other  hand, 
a  heightened  S  may  evoke  more  intense  reaction,  i,  from  associates  at 
time  of  contact.  This  will  have  the  consequence  of  increasing  the  refractory 
periods,  a,  and  thus  wath  frequency  of  contacts  maintained  harmonious 


J.  The  Social  Use  of  Space  131 

with  A^6,  the  thetas,  both  those  of  satiation  and  frustration,  will  be  increased. 
In  this  situation  the  critical  point  becomes  the  attitude  of  associates  toward 
such  indi\'iduals.  If  the  attitude  toward  this  individual  possessing  rarely- 
encountered  characteristics  is  one  in  which  the  desired  characteristic  is 
venerated  or  desired  by  the  majority  not  possessing  it,  then  the  resulting 
intense  interaction  will  have  the  consequence  of  more  frequently  resulting 
in  an  aaa  satiation  type  refractory  period.  However,  it  is  much  more  likely 
that  the  strange,  rare  characteristic  will  elicit  an  aggressive  or  rejection 
type  response  leading  to  an  aap  frustrating  type  refractory  period  for  the 
individual  with  the  heightened  S. 

When  the  iVi  subgroup  represents  a  majority,  they  can  achieve  a  return 
of  their  n  and  9's  to  more  nearly  normal  levels  by  ejecting  the  aberrant 
A''2  individuals.  However,  the  minority  A''2  members  of  the  A^i  +  A^2  group 
suffer  most  from  heightened  m  and  ^'s.  Therefore,  their  seeking  escape  from 
the  group  becomes  a  motivating  force  greater  than  that  of  ejection  by  their 
associates. 

If  neither  A^i  or  A''2  form  a  clear  majority,  the  most  likely  result  will  be 
a  splitting  of  the  group  in  half,  but  with  retention  of  both  A''!  and  A''2  type 
individuals  in  each  smaller  group.  This  consequence  derives  from  the 
following ; 

When  S  doubles,  nb  doubles,  the  new  n  =  2.0. 

The  "effort"  of  the  group  will  be  to  make  the  easiest  adjustment  which 
will  make  ^t  =  2.0  =  mo-  This  route  lies  in  reducing  the  A^  which  was  an 
Nb  to  an  No.  From  Eq.  (78)  it  follows  that: 

A^i       iV2       1 
A^o=l+^  +  ^--  (100) 

The  best  approximation  any  group  can  make  is  to  divide  in  half.  Each 
half  must  contain  nearly  equal  numbers  of  A^i  and  A''2.  If  all  A^i  formed  a 
group  spatially  distinct  from  the  iV2  members  of  the  former  Nb,  the  m  of 
every  member  in  each  group  would  automatically  return  to  1.0  since  within 
each  new  and  smaller  group  all  members  would  have  the  same  target 
diameter.  Thus,  with  the  A'"  of  each  new  group  being  only  O.oNb,  every 
member  would  experience  a  marked  reduction  in  satisfaction  and  frustra- 
tion thetas  below  optimum  since  the  frequency  of  contacts /c  [Ecj.  (34)] 
would  automatically  be  reduced. 

In  discussing  this  concept  of  a  few  divergent  individuals  or  even  a  single 
one  doubling  the  n  of  the  entire  group,  one  of  my  colleagues  remarked  that 
such  an  increase  appears  unreasonable.  Three  examples  will  suffice  to 
demonstrate  the  I'easonableness  of  this  assumption.  Barnett  (1955)  main- 


132  John  B.  Calhoun 

tained  established  groups  of  Norway  rats  in  large  cages.  Despite  or  because 
of  the  existing  hierarchy  characterizing  the  group,  all  rats  exhibited  rela- 
tive amicable  relations,  one  to  another.  All  presumably  had  developed 
nearly  identical  target  diameters.  Introduction  of  a  single  alien  rat  im- 
mediately produced  a  state  of  turmoil  within  the  group,  particularly  in- 
tense actions  being  directed  toward  the  alien  by  all  members  of  the  estab- 
lished group.  In  such  experiments  the  ahen  rat,  the  one  with  the  markedly 
differing  target  diameter,  frequently  died  within  a  day  or  two.  This  death 
came,  not  as  a  result  of  wounds  received,  but  as  a  result  of  inability  to 
accommodate  physiologically  to  the  intense  action  directed  toward  him. 
His  df  exceeded  a  threshold  compatible  with  survival. 

Or  we  may  take  any  one  of  the  several  incidents  publicized  by  the  press 
and  television  during  1960  and  1961  in  which  "Freedom  Riders"  engaged  in 
action  which  challenged  the  established  value  systems  of  certain  segments 
of  the  socially  dominant  whites  in  the  Deep  South  of  the  United  States. 
Such  actions  freciuently  evoke  drastic  reaction,  including  physical  \1o- 
lence,  from  members  of  the  established  group.  Furthermore,  many  cross 
currents  of  elevated  intensity  of  interaction  became  generated  within 
members  of  the  established  group  as  they  considered  appropriate  adjust- 
ments to  the  threat  to  their  held  values  and  to  their  prior  target  diameter. 

The  point  I  have  been  making  in  both  the  theoretical  formulation  and 
in  these  examples  is  that  a  qualitative  change  in  the  target  diameter  of  a 
portion  of  the  group  will  produce  both  an  increase  in  n  and  an  increase  in 
intensity  of  interaction.  Another  pertinent  example  is  one  I  have  previously 
given  (Calhoun,  1956,  pp.  87-88).  That  example  concerns  the  establish- 
ment of  a  new  group,  designated  as  "C57  Colony  IB,"  from  two  smaller 
groups  of  mice  not  previously  having  contact  with  each  other.  After  pro- 
vision of  an  access  door  through  the  wall  previously  separating  the  two 
groups,  the  males  from  the  socially  more  integrated  group  invaded  the 
living  space  of  the  other  males.  Not  only  did  the  former  attack  the  latter, 
but  the  males  from  the  more  socially  integrated  group  began  a  period  of 
intense  fighting  among  themselves  such  as  had  never  previously  been 
observed. 

Admittedly,  all  three  of  these  examples  include  introduction  of  aliens 
into  an  established  group  rather  than  the  hypothesized  divergence  in 
target  diameter  of  a  portion  of  a  single  group.  However,  it  is  difficult  to 
imagine  a  portion  of  a  group  altering  their  target  diameter  without  tempo- 
rary partial  isolation  from  the  remainder.  So,  in  effect,  the  consequences 
of  divergence  of  target  diameters  among  members  of  a  group  will  be  the 
same  regardless  of  how  the  group  is  assembled. 

In  actuality 

M  =  {Sv/A)  (101) 


1.  The  Social  Use  of  Space  133 

However,  in  most  instances,  I  shall  continue  to  consider  fj.  =  (dv/A)  as 
previously.  But  when  so  doing,  it  must  be  understood  that  d  is  used  in  the 
sense  of  its  S  response-evoking  capacity. 

When  S  changes  from  1.0  to  2.0  for  the  reasons  relating  to  Eq.  (99),  n 
will  no  longer  be  m6  =  (dv/A)  =  1.0,  but  /x  will  then  become  (2dv/A)  = 
2.0.  Inserting  fx  =  2.0  into  Eq.  (78),  in  which  fx'  remains  1.0,  for  the  special 
case  where  A^";,  =  12,  then  No  becomes  6.5.  This  means  that  in  the  presence 
of  conflicting  values  group  size  must  be  reduced  for  each  individual  to 
maintain  its  do,  its  optimum  level  of  satiation  from  social  interaction.  In- 
crease in  fi  follows  increases  in  d  or  v,  or  decrease  in  A.  Regardless  of  the 
origin  of  the  increase  in  /z,  reduction  in  group  size  should  follow. 

Such  reduction  in  group  size  should  not  be  instantaneous.  Consider 
Nb  =  12,  ab  =  0.091  and  n  =  2.0  and  the  group  has  not  yet  fragmented. 
From  Eq.  (82)  it  is  obvious  that  Nb  =  Ni,  when  Ho  =  2.0  and  a  =  ab, 
and  that  da,  the  deficit  in  satiation  from  social  interaction,  will  be  as  great 
as  if  /x  had  remained  unchanged  at  1 .0  and  Nb  increased  to  [1  +  2  ( A^6  —  1 )  ]. 
[Refer  to  Section  XIII,  B,  2.]  This  is  a  very  interesting  consequence  for 
it  means  that  when  n  increases  to  2.0,  Nb  =  Ni.  Recall  that  Ni  is  that  N 
at  which  an  increment  in  N  brings  about  the  greatest  change  in  dd.  Since 
groups  do  resist  division  and  since  any  increase  in  /x  is  likely  to  be  gradual, 
the  most  likely  time  for  fragmentation  of  the  group  is  when  fx  becomes  2.0 
and  Nb  =  Ni.  Then  Nb  will  divide  into  two  groups  approximating  No 
determined  by  Eq.  (78).  Roughly,  this  says  that  when  the  ease  of  com- 
munication doubles  as  a  result  of  a  doubling  of  the  response-evoking 
capacity  S,  the  group  will  approximately  divide  in  half  if  it  is  to  optimize 
satiation  from  social  interaction. 

This  process  of  halving  the  basic  group  size  each  time  the  ease  of  com- 
munication becomes  twdce  as  efficient  cannot  continue  long  if  Nb  =  12, 
because  by  the  fourth  doubling  of  fx,  sexual  reproduction  could  no  longer 
be  tolerated.  That  is,  No  would  be  less  than  two  individuals.  The  practice 
of  divorce  by  the  human  animal  reflects  this  process.  We  now  have  another 
question  raised:  "What  avenue  of  adaptation  or  adjustment  is  open  if  Nb 
remains  12  and  i  remains  unchanged  at  uV 

Although  Id  theoretically  may  be  defined  in  terms  of  attributes  of  d,  v, 
and  A  external  to  the  organism,  any  solution  to  this  question  demands 
that  n  must  in  effect  be  reduced  back  to  1.0  by  some  compensating 
mechanism. 

This  mechanism  which  alters  the  probability  of  a  contact  being  socially 
perceived  has  been  called  n'.  In  Eqs.  (38)  to  (55)  it  was  shown  that  mm' 
represents  the  appropriate  interaction  between  these  two  factors.  So  far 
/x'  has  been  elaborated  no  further,  /xju'  then  becomes  the  communication 
constant,  more  explicitly  stated  as  {dv/A)ix.  Since  m  can  vary  as  a  result 
of  any  one  of  its  contained  factors,  d,  v,  or  A,  fluctuating  alone,  one  cannot 


134  John  B.  Calhoun 

escape  the  conclusion  that  during  evolution  a  separate  compensating 
mechanism  for  each  must  have  arisen.  This  means  that  there  is  a  d',  a  v', 
and  an  A',  and  that  /jl'  =  id'v' / A').  Furthermore,  when  mm'  =  1-0,  [i  = 
\/\x.  Having  arrived  at  these  insights,  one  is  logically  lead  to  ask:  "What 
do  d\  v' ,  and  A'  most  likely  represent  biologically?"  Although  answering 
this  ciuestion  is  not  necessary  for  the  general  theoretical  formulations,  an 
attempt  to  specify  their  more  likely  nature  may  be  helpful  in  searching 
for  their  identification. 

d'  represents  the  degree  to  which  the  stimuli  emanating  from  any  con- 
figuration pass  unselectively  from  the  sense  organs  into  the  memory  store 
of  the  central  nervous  system  (CNS).  Thus,  an  increase  in  d'  means 
facilitation  of  passage  of  stimuli  into  the  CNS,  while  a  decrease  in  d'  indi- 
cates impeding  or  preventing  stimuli  from  getting  to  the  CNS.  When  the 
target  diameter  of  associates  increases  through  evolution  by  acquiring 
more  (/-genes,  a  compensating  evolution  of  a  d'-mechanism  wall  permit  a 
discrimination  among  the  <i-genes  such  that  in  that  brief  span  of  time  re- 
quired for  psychological  contact  only  a  portion  of  the  c?-genes  of  the  other 
individual  will  be  responded  to.  It  must  be  kept  clearly  in  mind  that  an  in- 
crease in  the  efficiency  of  the  mechanism  which  serves  to  alter  d' ,  decreases 
d'.  Such  a  decrease  in  d'  imphes  the  evolution  of  a  filtering  device  which  re- 
duces the  amount  of  information  about  others  per  unit  time  arriving  at 
the  sense  organs,  which  is  permitted  to  pass  from  them  into  the  integrative 
centers  of  the  nervous  system.  Without  specifying  what  CNS  structure 
serves  the  d'  function,  it  meets  the  reciuirements  hypothesized  by  Broad- 
bent  for  his  CNS  "filter."  See  Section  IX. 

v'  also  represents  a  process  internal  to  the  indi\'idual.  It  cannot  have 
any  influence  upon  the  motor  component  of  v.  Therefore  it  must  affect 
the  consequences  of  those  sensory  capacities  which  enable  the  individual 
to  achieve  a  psychological  contact  prior  to  an  actual  physical  one.  This  is 
the  r  component  of  velocity  mentioned  in  Section  XIII,  A,  1.  Just  as  an 
increase  in  r  increases  v  by  decreasing  the  time  required  for  a  contact,  so 
must  a  decrease  in  v'  function  to  increase  the  time  from  the  moment  of 
input  of  a  signal  from  a  d-gene  at  the  sensory  organ  until  this  transformed 
signal  reaches  and  evokes  a  response  at  an  effector.  Thus  v'  could  represent 
either  a  structural  or  biochemical  alteration  in  the  time  required  for  an 
impulse  to  pass  over  a  synapse,  or  it  could  be  represented  by  an  alteration 
in  the  number  of  neurons  in  the  circuit  which  will  also  alter  transmission 
time.  But  we  must  not  confuse  the  magnitude  of  v'  with  the  efficiency  of 
the  mechanism  involved.  An  increase  in  v'  means  a  decrease  in  the  efficiency 
of  the  mechanism,  that  is  increased  synaptic  transmission,  while  a  decrease 
in  v'  follows  from  an  increase  in  the  efficiency  of  the  mechanism  in  impeding 
the  passage  of  the  signal  along  the  circuit  between  the  sense  organ  to  the 
effectors. 


J.   The  Social  Use  of  Space  135 

A  word  is  required  to  differentiate  clearly  d'  from  v'.  d'  governs  the  prol)- 
ability  of  a  signal  relating  to  c?-genes  getting  through  the  Broadbent  typo 
hypothesized  "filter,"  while  v'  applies  to  the  speed  of  transmission  from  the 
filter  to  effectors. 

In  a  similar  fashion,  conceptualization  of  A'  must  be  in  terms  of  counter- 
acting A.  Where  an  A''^  group  is  living  under  optimum  conditions,  A  may 
be  considered  to  be  equal  to  1.0.  Optimum  conditions  will  continue  even 
though  A  changes  in  so  long  as  A  A'  =  1.0.  For  the  sake  of  simplicity  we 
are  considering  the  case  where  the  only  change  in  the  system  pertains  to 
A.  Recall  that  A  represents  the  area  which  the  group  shares.  In  essence 
then  A  alters  the  time  between  contacts.  Therefore  A'  must  operate  in  a 
similar  but  opposite  direction  to  .4.  Suppose  that  A  increases.  In  effect 
this  is  equivalent  to  a  decrease  in  density.  Under  such  circumstances  there 
can  be  no  internal  mechanism  enhancing  the  probability  of  an  actual 
contact.  Therefore,  when  A  increases,  an  A'  compensating  mechanism  in- 
volves an  imagined  contact.  To  the  extent  that  such  an  imagined  contact 
leads  to  an  equal  a  refractory  period,  an  A'  mechanism  will  be  effective. 
Since  an  increase  in  A  implies  a  decrease  in  A',  a  decrease  in  A'  means  an 
increase  in  the  capacity  to  store  memories  of  associates,  which  can  com- 
pensate for  their  absence.  Such  an  increased  storage  of  memories  implies 
an  increase  in  cortical  mass. 

On  the  other  hand,  suppose  that  A  decreases.  This  will  reduce  the  time 
elapsing  between  contacts,  and  since  this  will  have  the  same  consequence 
as  increasing  N  w^hen  we  are  concerned  with  an  Nh  group,  its  members 
will  experience  an  increased  da  and  an  increase  in  6/  above  the  optimum 
level.  In  this  situation  A'  must  function  to  increase  the  relative  time  be- 
tween contacts.  The  only  way  for  this  to  happen  is  for  the  intensity,  i,  of 
interaction  to  decrease.  Recall  that  intensity  of  interaction  has  been 
measured  in  terms  of  its  duration,  and  that  i  =  (a)^  when  a  represented 
the  duration  of  the  refractory  period  following  interaction.  Furthermore, 
Eq.  (60)  becomes: 

fJLfx'iN    -     1) 

when  we  consider  the  several  factors  in  the  general  sense  where  a  variation 
in  one  may  influence  any  other. 
Then 

an'  =  ^- (103) 

^         fxiN  -  1) 

The  reason  for  stating  the  equation  in  this  fashion  is  that  n'  cannot  alter 
/i  as  such  but  can  only  alter  its  effects  through  changing  something  else. 


136  John  B.  Calhoun 

What  Eq.  (103j  implies  is  that  where  A'  is  the  factor  which  produces  the 
variabihty  iu  /x',  it  can  be  effective  in  maintaining  60  but  not  necessarily 
an  optimum  6/  (see  discussion  following  Eq.  (74)),  provided  it  acts  as  a 
governor  on  the  intensity  of  interaction  independent  from  the  influence 
upon  i  exerted  by  d.  A  decrease  in  the  A  component  of  n  indicates  a  de- 
crease in  the  home  range  a  (see  Sections  II-V).  The  significance  of  this 
hne  of  reasoning  is  that  this  A  governor,  which  controls  i,  is  likely  to  be 
identical  to  the  one  previously  postulated  for  determining  the  duration  of 
an  outward  trip  from  home.  In  the  general  sense,  this  governor  controls 
the  duration  of  behaviors.  Social  interaction  merely  represents  one  specific 
category  of  behaviors.  Related  to  the  above  discussion,  it  may  be  noted 
that  Eqs.  (69)  and  (103)  are  equivalent  since  n'  =  l/n,  so  long  as  m  =  1-0 
or  fjL  >  1.0.  As  originally  formulated  in  the  discussion  before  Ec^.  (38),  n' 
simply  operated  as  a  probability  of  an  actual  contact  being  perceived. 
However,  the  above  and  following  discussion  indicate  that  m'  can  exceed 
1.0.  By  imagining  contacts,  which  actually  do  not  occur,  through  a  de- 
crease of  the  A'  component  of  /i',  n'  can  exceed  1.0. 

In  terms  of  the  model  proposed  in  Section  III,  A,  whenever  A  decreases, 
the  rate  of  firing  of  the  neuronal  net  of  the  governor  will  increase.  Thus, 
an  increase  in  A'  represents  an  increased  rate  of  firing  of  the  neuronal  net 
of  the  governor. 

Previously  I  pointed  out  that  a  decrease  in  A'  to  compensate  for  an  in- 
crease in  A  might  be  visualized  as  an  hallucinatory  process.  There  is  no 
reason  why  A'  camiot  equally  involve  the  governor  of  intensity  of  activity 
when  A  increases  above  .-l^  normally  appropriate  to  Nb',  a  decrease  in  A' 
would  represent  a  slowing  of  the  firing  of  the  neuronal  net  of  the  governor, 
which  change  would  then  permit  a  longer  duration  of  interaction.  This 
longer  duration  of  interaction  would  compensate  for  the  fewer  /««  interac- 
tions possible  in  a  larger  A. 

The  concept  of  social  perception  of  contacts  subsumed  under  n'  thus 
includes  a  wdde  variety  of  processes.  It  includes  (1)  selective  acceptance  of 
stimuli  to  be  integrated  in  the  central  nervous  system;  (2)  impedance  or 
facilitation  of  passage  of  signals  across  neuronal  synapses;  (3)  alteration 
in  the  length  of  a  neuronal  circuit;  (4)  hallucinating  a  contact;  and  (5) 
governing  the  intensity  of  interaction.  These  m'  capacities  for  adjustment 
are  most  likely  to  be  found  farthest  advanced  in  those  species  in  which  d, 
V,  and  A  fluctuate  most  markedly  with  reference  to  all  members  of  a  group 
within  the  life  span  of  every  individual.  However,  over  long  spans  of  time 
encompassing  many  generations,  a  gradual  increase  in  d,  v,  and  A  should 
increase  the  complexity  of  social  life  possible.  At  least  this  conclusion 
holds  to  the  extent  that  enhanced  synaptic  transmission,  increased  dura- 
tion and  intensity  of  behavior,  and  increased  discriminatory  capacity 
foster  more  effective  social  behavior. 


1.  The  Social  Use  of  Space 


137 


The  following  summary  of  presumed  relationships  may  serve  as  a  basis 
for  evaluating  the  consequences  of  change  in  the  components  of  ju  and  m'- 


External 

Internal 

change 

change 

Increase  in 

Decrease  in 

d 

d' 

V 

v' 

A 

A' 

=  Increase  in  discriminatory  capacity 

=  Impeded  synaptic  transmission 

=  Increased    duration    and    intensity    of    be- 
haviors, or 
Increased  memory  storage  of  social  stimuli 
permitting  hallucinated  interactions  which 
indicate  increase  in  cortical  mass 


Decrease  in     Increase  in 
d  d' 


=  Decreased  discriminatory  capacity  (i.e., 
less  selection  of  stimuli  passing  the 
"filter") 

V  v'  =  Enhanced  synaptic  transmission 

A  A'  =  Decreased   duration   and   intensity   of    be- 

havior, or  decreased  memory  storage  of 
stimuli  which  are  available  for  producing 
hallucinated  social  interactions  which  in- 
dicates decrease  in  cortical  mass 

These  relationships  represent  intuitive  logical  deductions,  d'  and  A' 
serve  as  fairly  satisfactory  first  approximations.  With  progressive  social 
evolution,  d  and  A  generally  increase.  Along  with  this  trend,  discriminatory 
capacity  increases,  cortical  mass  increases,  and  ability  to  maintain  a  be- 
havior for  longer  periods  of  time  increases,  all  of  which  tend  to  harmonize 
with  the  model.  However,  I  am  less  satisfied  with  my  formulation  of  v' 
(Section  XIII,  A,  1).  The  reason  for  this  opinion  is  that  with  advancing 
social  evolution  there  presumably  arises  an  increase  in  Vi,  that  sensory  ex- 
tension of  the  seK  to  produce  psychological  contact  before  bodily  contact. 
This  should  produce  the  opposing  phenomenon  of  impeded  synaptic  trans- 
mission. For  the  present,  I  can  see  no  rational  basis  for  reconciling  impeded 
synaptic  transmission  with  social  advances. 

v'  merely  connotes  alteration  of  sensory  perceptions  of  others  involving 
some  distance  between  the  two  individuals  concerned.  My  supposition 
that  alteration  of  synaptic  transmission  subserves  v'  may  well  be  wrong. 
The  important  point  for  the  present  is  simply  recognition  of  the  likelihood 
of  some  such  mechanism  whose  evolution  is  influenced  by  social  conditions. 


138  John  B.  Calhoun 

I  have  pointed  out  that  since  fx  can  change  as  a  result  of  independent 
change  in  d,  v,  and  A,  conseciuently  there  must  be  discrete  phenomena  in 
the  individual  representing  what  I  call  d',  v' ,  and  A' ,  which  can  change 
independent  of  each  other.  Nevertheless,  it  is  ciuite  likely  that  the  compo- 
nents of  n  and  /x'  do  have  interactions.  The  concept  of  intensity  (duration) , 
i,  of  social  action  includes  control  of  i  by  both  internal  and  external  factors. 
The  internal  factor  is  A',  the  "governor"  previously  discussed.  The  ex- 
ternal factor  is  the  d  of  the  other.  The  greater  d,  the  greater  i.  Thus,  d 
can  influence  the  governor.  This  means  that  an  increase  in  the  d  of  associ- 
ates can  decrease  the  A'  of  self. 

In  studies  with  rats  recently  completed  (Calhoun,  1962b)  some  rats 
develop  a  high  v  while  others  develop  a  low  v.  Those  with  a  very  high  v 
exhibit  high  i  in  terms  of  both  intensity  and  duration.  This  suggests  that 
in  some  way  an  increase  in  v  leads  to  a  decrease  in  A' .  Furthermore,  male 
rats  with  very  low  v  commonly  respond  as  though  they  did  not  make  ade- 
quate discrimination  of  the  cues  emanating  from  associates.  They  sexually 
mount  associates  without  regard  to  their  age,  sex,  or  sexual  receptivity. 
These  observations  suggest  that  lowering  of  the  motor  components  of  self's 
V  increases  self's  d' ,  which  suggests  that  somehow  when  an  animal  decreases 
its  velocity  its  ability  to  discriminate  among  available  social  stimuli  also 
becomes  reduced.  All  I  have  attempted  to  do  in  the  preceding  paragraphs 
is  to  lay  the  groundwork  for  understanding  the  meaning  of  the  contact- 
modifying  factor  jj.' . 

Decreases  of  n  re  increase  in  N :  We  are  here  concerned  with  the  special 
case  where  the  area.  A,  remains  constant  as  numbers  of  individuals,  N, 
increase.  This  means  that  density  increases.  We  have  already  seen  that  an 
increase  in  A^  with  ^  held  constant  leads  to  a  deficit,  da,  in  satisfaction  from 
social  interaction  as  well  as  an  increase  above  optimum  of  the  frustration, 
df,  from  such  interaction.  As  density  increases  one  should  anticipate  n 
changing  before  y.' .  Therefore  we  shall  consider  ix'  as  remaining  constant 
at  the  1.0  value  appropriate  to  Nb  but  let  N  increase.  In  each  instance  we 
wish  A^  to  become  No,  which  means  that  do  and  6/"'^  will  be  optimum.  Con- 
sider the  case  where  A^b  =  12  and  intensity  of  interaction  remains  at  u,  it 
may  be  seen  from  Eq.  (80)  that  successive  doublings  of  No  demands  suc- 
cessively slightly  more  than  halving  of  jXo'- 


No 


IJ'O 


12 

1.0 

24 

0.478 

48 

0.234 

96 

0.116 

192 

0.057 

384 

0.028 

1.  The  Social  Use  of  Space  139 

Where  fx  =  Ho  and  N  =  No  it  may  be  seen  by  substituting  Eq.  (80)  into 
Eq.  i'-U)  that  the  frequency  of  contacts  will  remain  constant  at  (Nb  —  1) 
regardless  of  the  increase  in  density.  Since  we  are  considering  the  special 
case  where  area,  A,  remains  constant,  the  constancy  in  the  absolute  num- 
ber of  contacts  despite  increases  in  density  can  only  derive  from  decreases 
in  the  d  or  the  v  component  of  ju  =  dv/A. 

Where  decreases  in  ju  transpire  within  the  lifetime  of  an  individual  as  an 
adjustive  change  to  increase  in  density,  and  where  this  decrease  in  m  results 
solely  from  a  decrease  in  the  target  diameter,  d,  this  change  must  be  re- 
flected through  reductions  in  the  frequency  of  usage  of  signahng  mechanisms 
or  of  the  intensity  of  such  signals.  Vocalizations  should  occur  less  frequently, 
be  less  complex,  and  of  reduced  intensity.  Bodily  display  characteristics 
under  voluntary  control  should  similarly  be  reduced,  as  should  also  the 
use  of  chemical  signals.  Similarly,  when  a  species,  A,  now  exhibits  an  Nb 
of  80-120  or  800-1400  (see  Section  XIII,  B,  3)  but  there  is  reason  to  sus- 
pect that  at  some  earlier  era  it  had  had  an  Nb  of  2  or  12,  such  as  is  still 
expressed  by  a  related  species  (or  genus),  B,  then  a  comparative  examina- 
tion of  species  of  types  A  and  B  should  reveal  that  members  of  species  B 
more  frequently  resort  to  the  utilization  of  vocal,  display,  and  chemical 
signals  and  that  these  are  of  greater  complexity  and  intensity  than  in 
species  A.  In  making  any  such  comparisons  it  is  well  to  keep  in  mind  the 
earlier  hypothesis  that  the  change  of  an  Nb  =  2  to  an  Nb  =  12  may  lead 
to  an  increase  in  complexity  of  the  signal.  For  this  reason,  it  is  more  ap- 
propriate to  compare  a  species  w^hose  A^6  has  a  typical  range  of  7-19  with 
one  which  rarely  falls  below  80.  Such  comparisons  will  provide  critical 
tests  of  the  general  formulation. 

In  like  fashion,  velocity,  v,  may  be  the  factor  which  becomes  reduced  as 
density  increases.  This  reduction  may  be  expressed  in  any  of  the  three 
aspects  of  v  listed  in  Section  XIII,  A,  1.  Sense  organs  may  become  less 
effective  with  reference  to  the  animals'  perception.  This  reduces  ^i.  Such 
a  tactic  must  be  effective  only  through  genetic  changes  and  so  can  serve 
only  as  a  long-term  adaptive  mechanism.  The  animal  may  also  reduce  its 
V2  by  w^ay  of  reducing  the  number  of  trips  it  makes,  or  by  altering  the  time 
and  place  of  its  activities.  This  strategy  includes  initiating  activity  while 
others  are  resting,  or  by  becoming  cryptic  in  the  sense  of  becoming  arboreal 
or  subterranean.  Finally,  the  animal  may  reduce  its  Vs,  its  actual  rate  of 
movement. 

Although  such  reductions  in  d  and  v  will  lead  to  a  reduction  in  m  which 
will  compensate  for  increase  in  density,  it  is  obvious  that  n  must  decline 
so  markedly  as  to  present  biological  limits  of  effectiveness.  Further  accom- 
modation to  increases  in  density  must  require  m'  to  decrease  also.  Reduc- 
tion in  this  communication-inhibiting  or  contact-blinding  factor  means 
(a)   decreased  duration  or  intensity  of  behaviors  or  decreased  memory 


140  John  B.  Calhoun 

storage  (i.e.,  increase  in  A');  (b)  increase  in  discriminatory  power  in  the 
sense  of  screening  out  portions  of  those  stimuli  of  the  d  of  others  requisite 
for  ehciting  responses  (i.e.,  decreasing  d');  or  (c)  impeded  synaptic  trans- 
mission (i.e.,  decreasing  v'). 

5.  Behavioral  Origin  of  Response-Evoking  Capacity,  S 

a.  The  target  diameter  genotype  as  determined  by  varibility  of  behavioral 
traits.  I  now  wish  to  present  the  logic  of  why  variabiHty  of  behavioral 
traits  becomes  inevitable.  In  fact,  as  aminals  become  more  social,  varia- 
bility in  physical  traits  must  become  of  less  importance  in  determining 
the  kind  and  intensities  of  interaction.  So  let  us  start  with  the  case  where 
all  indi\'iduals  possess  identical  heredity  and  therefore  identical  physical 
characteristics.  Even  for  so  simple  an  organism  as  the  house  mouse,  marked 
differences  in  capacities  for  social  involvement  develop  despite  the  fact 
that  the  members  of  the  group  come  from  a  stock  made  genetically  homo- 
zygous by  nearly  a  hundred  generations  of  brother-to-sister  inbreeding 
(Calhoun,  1956). 

The  initial  formulation  of  social  interaction  dealt  with  a  deterministic 
model  in  which  all  individuals  w^ere  identical.  It  showed  that  half  the  time 
an  individual  was  in  the  responsive  state  it  would  meet  another  responsive 
individual  and  half  the  time  it  would  meet  another  in  the  refractory  state. 
Thus,  even  under  ideal  conditions,  an  individual  would  be  frustrated  as 
frequently  as  it  would  be  satisfied  from  social  interaction.  But  satisfaction 
will  not  hkely  precisely  alternate  with  frustration.  Furthermore,  if  we 
consider  some  arbitrary  relatively  short  span  of  time  when  the  group  first 
forms,  determined  by  the  average  individual  having,  for  example,  a  total 
pool  of  interactions  equivalent  to  2-5  times  the  number  of  individuals  in 
the  group,  then  something  like  the  following  will  have  transpired : 

Each  individual's  behavior  toward  another  may  be  characterized  by  its 
form  or  pattern  and  by  its  timing  with  regard  to  whether  the  other  in- 
dividual involved  in  the  interaction  is  also  in  the  responsive  state  (the  a 
state)  or  whether  it  is  in  the  opposite  or  nonresponsive  state  (the  p  state) . 
Initially  the  form  of  the  behavior  of  all  individuals  in  the  responsive  state 
will  be  identical.  Identical  form  denotes  possession  of  the  entire  assembly 
of  traits,  d,  by  every  individual.  With  each  individual  contacting  its  as- 
sociates in  a  random  sequence  over  time,  it  is  inevitable  that  some,  who 
are  in  the  responsive  a  state,  will  purely  by  chance  more  frequently  en- 
counter others  who  happen  to  be  in  the  nonresponsive  p  state.  Each  such 
encounter  will  throw  the  responsive  individual  into  an  a^^  frustrating  type 
refractory  period.  Thus,  the  appropriate  behavior  of  this  individual  will 
not  only  not  be  rewarded,  it  w^ill  actually  be  punished.  After  this  individual 


1.  The  Social  Use  of  Space  141 

passes  through  its  a^p  refractory  period  and  again  enters  the  responsive 
state,  some  random  change  may  typify  its  behavior.  The  more  freciuently 
it  is  frustrated,  the  more  hkely  will  its  behavior  become  deviant  simply 
because  there  has  been  so  infrequent  reinforcement  of  its  original  appropri- 
ate form.  Conversely,  those  individuals  which,  when  they  are  in  the  re- 
sponsive state,  have  met  another  also  in  the  responsive  state,  will  have  the 
original  appropriate  behavior  rewarded  or  reinforced.  The  behavior  of 
such  individuals  will  remain  much  in  its  original  form. 

In  this  way  the  members  of  a  group  may  be  rank  ordered  according  to 
the  degree  to  which  their  behavior  has  deviated  from  the  original.  Each 
recognizable  unit  of  deviation  represents  a  recessive  d-gene.  Each  unit  of 
retention  of  the  original  behavior  pattern  from  which  theirs  has  been  a 
deviation  represents  a  dominant  c?-gene.  The  more  dominant  c?-genes  an 
individual  possesses,  the  more  intensely  will  associates  respond  to  him  in  a 
positive  affective  manner,  and  the  more  likely  will  he  be  chosen  as  a 
partner  or  leader.  Conversely,  the  more  recessive  c?-genes  an  individual 
possesses,  the  more  intensely  will  associates  impose  restraints  or  sanctions 
on  him,  and  the  more  likely  will  they  reject  him. 

It  is  useful  to  borrow  terminology  from  genetics  which  deals  with  he- 
redity. Let  upper  case  letters  represent  dominant  d-genes,  and  lowercase 
letters  represent  recessive  d-genes.  Such  a  system  for  an  A^  =  11  is  shown 
in  Table  XIII.  Here  (d)  represents  the  common  traits  shared  by  all  mem- 
bers of  N.  (d)  plus  the  remaining  dominant  and  recessive  genes  specify 
an  individual's  rf-genotype.  That  individual  which  has  been  exposed  to  the 
least  number  of  circumstances  producing  changes  in  its  c?-genotype  may 
be  said  to  possess  the  "ideal"  d-genotype.  All  other  individuals  will  diverge 
more  or  less  from  this  ideal.  They  may  be  rank  ordered  from  the  alpha  in- 
dividual with  the  ideal  d-genotype  to  the  omega  nth  ranked  individual, 
which  differs  most  from  the  alpha  one.  Each  can  then  be  assigned  a  simi- 
larity rank,  R.  The  alpha  individual  is  represented  by  Ri;  the  one  who 
differs  least  from  the  alpha  has  R2;  while  the  individual  who  differs  most 
from  the  alpha  has  Rn.  In  Eqs.  (92)  and  (93)  i  =  R  (Tables  XIII  to  XV). 

Consider  R4.  By  utilizing  Eq.  (93)  its  c?-genotype,  by  which  we  simply 
mean  its  d,  becomes: 

d  =  (d)  +  gi'^  +  r/r-  +  r/;^  +  g\,''  +  g'/' 

+  g'o''  +  gk''  +  g'l''  +  g'^    (104) 

Obviously  the  d  for  each  similarity  rank,  R,  will  be  different  for  every 
other  one.  Therefore  S  for  each  individual  will  be  unique.  This  response- 
evoking  capacity,  S,  represents  the  d-phenotype. 


142 


John  B.  Calhoun 


Table  XIII 
Trait,  d-GENE,  Differentiation  in  a  Similarity  Rank  Hierarchy 


Common 
Similarity        N^        Nj  traits 

rank,  R  {d) 


Differentiating  traits,  d-genes 


10 


11 


11  1 


2         10 


id) 


Dominant  d-genes 


ABCDEFGHI   J 


10     2      (d)     a 


B   CDEFGHI   J 


9    3      (d)     a   b 


CDEFGHI   J 


S    4      (rf)     a   b   c 


D  E   F   G   H  I   J 


7     5      {(})  abed 


E  F   G  H  I   J 


G     6       ((/)     a   I)   c   d   e 


F   G  H  I   J 


id)  a   b   c   d   e   f   G   H   I   J 


4     8       id')  a   b   c   d   e   f   g 


H  I   J 


3     9      {d)  a   b   c   d   e   f   g   h 


I   J 


(d)  a   b   c   d   e   f   g   h   i  I  J 


1    11      {d)  a   b   c   d   e   f   g   h   i   j 


Recessive  d-genes 


1.  The  Social  Use  of  Space  143 

b.  Response-evoking  capacities.  Prior  formulations  regarding  the  typing  of 
behavior  and  personality,  exemplified  by  Schaefer  (19r)9,  1961),  suggest  that 
the  nature  of  one's  own  S  and  the  nature  of  the  response  evoked  from  others 
is  influenced  by  the  mood  or  attitude  of  the  other  individual  involved.  This 
mood  or  attitude  determines  how  one  views  the  target  diameters  of  others. 
It  determines  whether  one  focuses  on  the  dominant  rf-genes  or  the  recessive 
<:/-genes  of  others.  That  is,  when  an  individual  responds  to  another  does  he 
look  at  the  other's  desirable  or  undesirable  characteristics.  It  will  be  a 
prime  premise  of  my  formulation  that  one  can  be  afTected  only  by  the  good 
or  by  the  bad  side  of  another  at  a  particular  moment  in  time,  but  not  by 
both  simultaneously.  Furthermore,  I  shall  show  that  there  are  two  ways  of 
assessing  the  good  qualities,  the  dominant  d-genes,  of  another.  Likewise, 
there  are  two  ways  of  assessing  the  undesirable  finalities,  the  recessive 
rf-genes,  of  another.  Any  individual's  (/-genotype  in  a  social  setting  produces 
four  types  of  r/-phcnotypos,  that  is  four  kinds  of  S. 

Let 

*S(-^>  represent  the  response-e\-oking  capacity  of  an  individual  affecting 
the  probability  of  his  being  chosen  by  associates  as  an  object  of 
affection.  Sa  is  related  to  Schaefer's  love-acceptance.  Sa  depends 
upon  both  the  common  target  diameter,  (d) ,  and  on  dominant 
d-genes. 

gia)  represent  the  response-evoking  capacity  of  an  individual  affecting 
the  probability  of  his  being  rejected  by  associates.  S'^"^  is  related 
to  Schaefer's  hostility-rejection.  S'-"^  depends  solely  on  recessive 
rf-genes.  It  is  dependent  in  no  way  upon  the  common  traits,  (d) . 

S'^^^  represent  the  response-evoking  capacity  of  an  individual  affecting 
how  intensely  he  will  be  loved  or  approved  of  by  associates.  Love 
here  implies  intensity  of  positive  response  and  thus  connotes 
increase  in  probability  of  an  individual  realizing  the  objective  of 
his  behavior.  *S'^'^  determines  the  extent  to  which  an  individual's 
desires  will  be  facilitated  by  associates.  In  this  sense,  S'-^^  is 
related  to  Schaefer's  autonomy.  S'-^^  depends  upon  both  the 
common  target  channels,  (d),  and  on  dominant  rf-genes. 

*§(")  represent  the  response-evoking  capacity  of  an  individual  affecting 
the  degree  to  which  its  velocity  is  altered.  S'-'^  is  related  to 
Schaefer's  control.  *S^''^  is  dependent  solely  upon  recessive  d- 
genes.  It  influences  the  intensity  of  negative  sanctions  imposed  by 
associates. 

Equations  for  ;S^^'  and   S'f''  have  already  been  given;  i.e.,  Eqs.  (92) 
and  (93)  subject  to  the  restrictions  there  stated. 


144  John  B.  Calhoun 

Let: 
Pi^^  =  the  probability  of  choosing  any  ith  individual. 
Pi"^  =  the  probability  of  rejecting  any  zth  individual. 


Then 


Vr  =  -W^ (105) 


Vr  =  -W^—  (106) 

i  here,  and  in  Eqs.  (92)  and  (93)  and  (105)  to  (113),  refers  to  specifica- 
tion of  individuals  by  similarity  rank,  R.  See  discussion  before  Eq.  (104). 

Equations  (105)  and  (106)  in  essence  state  that  the  probability  of  any 
other  individual  choosing  or  rejecting  the  ith.  individual  depends  upon 
what  proportion  of  the  total  dominant  d-gene  pool,  or  recessive  c?-gene 
pool,  of  the  entire  N  individuals  is  encompassed  by  this  ith.  individual. 
Note  that  these  equations  include  evaluation  of  one's  entire  experience 
with  members  of  the  group,  including  awareness  of  one's  own  traits.  This 
topic  of  self-awareness  will  be  discussed  later. 

Conceptually,  it  is  somewhat  more  difficult  to  understand  S^^^  and  S^''\ 
although  the  equations  for  their  calculation  are  rather  simple.  Let  us  con- 
sider iS^"^  first,  since  earlier  reference  simply  to  S  was  usually  in  the  re- 
stricted sense  of  ^S'^''^ 

Consider  the  individual  in  Table  XIII  with  similarity  rank  6,  Re.  When 
individuals  Ri  to  R^  are  in  that  state  where  they  tend  to  impose  restraints 
or  sanctions  on  others,  they  will  view  Re  as  being  more  different  from  the 
ideal  type  than  they  themselves  are.  In  this  sense,  Ri  to  ^5  are  type  1 
individuals,  in  the  sense  of  Eq.  (94).  Similarly,  R^  to  Rn  will  perceive  ^7 
as  being  like  themselves  in  that  they  all  share  the  recessive  rf-gene,  g[^K 
Thus,  they  along  with  Re  may  be  considered  as  type  2  individuals,  in  the 
sense  of  Eq.  (95),  with  reference  to  calculating  the  S^"^  of  Re  by  Eq.  (97). 
Nj  for  Re  is  6.  By  a  similar  logic  the  S'^'"'>  of  each  individual  may  be  calcu- 
lated. See  Table  XV  for  S'^"^  calculated  for  every  member  of  an  A^  =  11 
as  depicted  in  Table  XIII. 

Each  individual  will  belong  to  a  different-sized  A''  of  type  2  indi^nduals. 
This  N  will  hereafter  be  referred  to  as  Nj  to  differentiate  it  from  the  A^2 
given  in  Eq.  (97) .  By  analogy  to  Eq.  (97)  : 

^(.-)  =  N/Ni  (107) 


1.  The  Social  Use  of  Space  145 

And  from  Eq.  (91)  it  follows  that 

Vi  =  N-JN  (108) 

Equation  (108)  has  proved  a  most  useful  one  in  the  study  of  social 
groups  of  experimental  animals  because  it  leads  to  predicting  the  degrees 
of  social  withdrawal  expected  among  any  group  of  known  size.  In  the  dis- 
cussion following  Eq.  (99)  I  pointed  out  that  where  /x  increases  as  a  result 
of  S^"^  becoming  greater  than  1.0,  acconmiodation  might  be  through 
ejection  of  those  members  with  the  largest  ^S^"^  or  by  a  splitting  of  the 
group.  Each  of  these  possibilities  presumes  unused  area  A  into  which  the 
appropriate  individuals  may  immigrate.  However,  when  surrounding 
groups  maintain  territories,  or  other  circmnstances  preclude  emigration, 
then  the  A  component  of  m  =  {Sv/A)  remains  constant.  Thus,  reduction 
in  velocity,  v,  becomes  the  only  avenue  for  reducing  /x  back  to  the  1.0 
value  appropriate  to  No. 

By  a  similar  line  of  reasoning  to  that  leading  to  Eci.  (107),  A''j  represents 
the  number  of  individuals  with  which  the  individual  in  question  possesses 
a  given  uniqueness  of  dominant  c?-genes. 

Reference  to  Table  XIII  will  clarify  the  meaning  of  Nj.  For  example, 
Rh  belongs  to  an  iVj  =  5  since  it  may  be  recognized  by  sharing  the  domi- 
nant c?-gene,  E,  wdth  four  other  individuals.  A^j  and  the  similarity  rank,  R, 
will  always  have  identical  numerical  values. 

By  analogy  to  Eq.  (107) 

S^P  =  N/Nj  (109) 

And  although  I  do  not  for  the  present  see  how  one  identifies  T'  in  biological 
or  social  terms,  although  it  may  represent  the  seeking  for  positive  affec- 
tion, it  is  obvious  that 

Vi  =  Nj/N  (110) 

In  this  sense  behavioral  c?-genes  do  not  represent  retention  or  deviation 
from  specific  behaviors.  Characterization  by  two  individuals  of  possessing 
at  least  three  degrees  of  deviation  does  not  mean  that  these  degrees  of 
deviation  are  identical. 

Now  consider  a  group  consisting  of  four  individuals,  the  pertinent  data 
and  calculations  for  which  are  given  in  Table  XIV.  The  probabilities  of 
the  dominant  and  recessive  c?-genes  are: 

Pa  =  1/4  Pa  =  3/4 

Pb  =  2/4  p,   -  2/4 

PC  =   3/4  p.  =  1/4 

Pid)  =  4/4 


146 


John  B.  Calhoun 


o 

f^    . 

tc 

fa     z 

°    S 

CU      ID 

^      J 

O     C 

«  o 

O 

^   w 

-<  > 

z  9 

H-c   n 

z   ^ 

O    tc 

>   K 

a   H 

'^    z 

^     H 

►J    s 

>■ 

-<   <; 

t— t 

PS   CL, 

>A 

o 

pa 

(-1   K, 

>  5 
-!; 

K    to 

a   « 

'-^   a 

C     Ch 

£-1    ei- 

z< 

2  M 

H     Z 

<     O 

z    - 

fi  K 

H    t 

S 

o 

■» 

>. 

a 

K 

H 

o 

u . 

t3 

o 

^ 

"OQ* 

S:^     < 


^ 


o 

o 

m 

« 

< 

33 

o 


Pi 


o 


1.  The  Social  Use  of  Space 


147 


The  pool  of  shared  behaviors,  (d),  acts  as  a  single  dominant  d-gene. 
Where  R  represents  the  rank  of  the  animal  in  terms  of  the  degree  of  reten- 
tion of  dominant  d-genes,  that  is  how  closely  it  expresses  the  ideal  proto- 
type manner  of  behaving,  S'-'^^  and  S'-"^  may  be  calculated  by  Eqs.  (92) 
and  (93)  utilizing  the  above  probabilities  of  dominant  and  recessive 
d-genes. 

(4/1  +  4/2  +  4/3  +  4/4)  =     8.33 
(4/2  +  4/3  +  4/4)  =     4.33 

(4/3  +  4/4)  =     2.33 

(4/4)  =     1.00 


Then 


^R=4 


16.00  =  4A^ 


Restatement  of  the  concept  of  behavioral  c?-genes  in  the  context  of  a 
hypothetical  example  of  their  relationship  to  the  origin  of  the  various 
stimulus-evoking  capacities  and  of  velocity,  v,  will  assist  in  clarifying  the 
relationships.  As  described  in  Section  XIII,  B,  5,  a  the  members  of  an  es- 
tablished group  may  be  ranked  in  terms  of  the  degree  to  which  deviation 
has  developed  from  the  ideal  prototype.  This  ideal  prototype  will  always 
be  that  individual  characterized  by  the  least  absolute  deviation.  There  will 
be  A^  —  1  degrees  of  deviation  in  a  group  of  N  individuals. 


Let: 

Ai, 
Then: 


S2,  •••,  Sn-1  represent  the  characterization  of  an  individual  by  1, 

2,  ■  ■  • ,  N  —  1  degrees  of  deviation 
A2,  •  •  • ,  A.v_i  represent  the  absence  of  de\'iations  5i,  82,  •  •  • ,  5.v_i 


5i  =  recessive  d-gene  a 

82  —  di  =  recessive  d-gene  b 

8;i  —  82  =  recessive  rf-gene  c 
etc. 


And 


Then 


Ai             =  dominant  d-gene  A 

A2  —  Ai  =  dominant  d-gene  B 

A3  —  A2  =  dominant  c?-gene  C 

etc. 

^R=l 

=    0 

C(a)       _ 

(4/3)                            =     1.33 

C(n)       _ 
^R=i    — 

(4/3  -h  4/2)                =     2.33 

S'r%    = 

(4/3  +  4/2  -K  4/1)   =     6.33 

j:  .s,^«) 

10.00  =  3A^  - 

ff=i 

(A^/2) 


148  John  B.  Calhoun 

N.1  and  A^j  in  the  sense  of  type  one  and  type  two  individuals  discussed 
in  relation  to  Eqs.  (94)  and  (95)  and  (107)  are  given  in  Table  XIV.  When 
A^j  =  2  it  means  that  the  second  ranked,  R  =  2,  individual  is  a  member  of 
a  subgroup  of  two  individuals  which  share  the  dominant  d-gene  B.  Like- 
wise when  N j  =  2  it  means  that  the  third  ranked,  R  =  3,  individual  be- 
longs to  a  subgroup  of  two  individuals  each  of  whom  possesses  recessive 
d-gene  b.  Every  individual  belongs  to  a  unique  A^j  and  N,.  Given  these 
uniquenesses  of  A^'j  and  A^j  as  shown  in  Table  XIV,  the  respective  S^^\ 
Sl"^  and  Vi  were  calculated  respectively  by  Eqs.  (109),  (107),  and  (108) 
and  presented  in  Table  XIV. 

Up  to  the  present  I  have  intentionally  maintained  the  discussion  of 
response-evoking  capacity,  S,  on  a  nearly  strictly  theoretical  plane  for 
the  purpose  of  clarifying  concepts.  Even  though  such  theoretical  formula- 
tions may  be  justified  in  their  own  right,  regardless  of  how  well  they  ap- 
proximate reality,  still  it  is  desirable  to  ascertain  whether  they  are  in 
harmony  with  observed  data.  To  this  end  I  selected  two  sets  of  empirically 
obtained  data.  One  involves  measurements  for  mice  from  which  velocity, 
V,  may  be  derived.  The  second  involves  the  choosing  of  table  partners 
among  groups  of  delinquent  girls.  These  latter  data  permit  determination 
of  how  well  the  observed  choosing  can  be  predicted  by  Eq.  (105). 


XIV.  Consequences  and  Examples  of  Social  Interaction  Systems 

A,  Velocity  Reduction  in  a  Hierarchy  of  Mice 

I  have  previously  described  (Calhoun,  1956)  the  patterns  of  social  rela- 
tionships which  develop  among  members  of  small  groups  of  inbred  domes- 
ticated mice.  Whenever  two  mice  passed  within  a  few  inches  of  each  other 
they  were  recorded  as  having  a  contact,  regardless  of  whether  or  not  a 
detectable  social  interaction  could  be  detected.  During  a  "contact"  two 
mice  merely  came  within  that  range  of  each  other  for  which  contentions 
for  status  did  at  times  develop.  We  shall  consider  the  example  provided 
by  a  group  of  eleven  C57  black,  inbred  male  mice  which  had  developed  a 
stable  hierarchy  prior  to  recording  the  frequency  and  kind  of  associations 
of  each  individual  with  his  associates.  Some  pairs  of  mice  contacted  each 
other  much  more  frequently  than  anticipated  on  a  chance  basis,  while 
members  of  other  pairs  very  infrequently  met.  These  mice  are  rank  ordered 
in  Table  XV  according  to  the  number  of  contacts  each  had  with  associates. 
For  all  practical  purposes  this  order  also  represents  the  observed  ability 
to  dominate  an  encounter  which  precipitated  in  a  fight  or  flight.  That  is. 


1.   The  Sdicial  Use  of  Space  149 

the  most  active  mouse  was  the  most  dominant  one  and  the  least  active 
one  the  most  subordinate. 

It  can  readily  be  shown  that  with  N's  as  large  as  eleven,  the  number  of 
contacts,  ric,  is  so  nearly  proportional  to  velocity,  v,  as  to  justify  utilizing 
number  of  contacts  as  a  measure  of  velocity  in  the  present  example.  From 
Eq.  (91)  it  is  seen  that  S^i'''>  varies  inversely  with  Vi,  and  therefore  on  a 
theoretical  basis  mice  with  few  contacts  should  be  those  wdth  large  response- 
evoking  capacities. 


Table  XV 
Velocity  in  a  Hierarchy  of  Mice 


1 

2 

3 

4 

5                         6 

7 

Rank 
R 

iVi 

Number  of  contacts,  ric,  expected 

Mouse's 
number 

Obs. 

Exp.                        V 

Eq.  (113)             Eq.  (108) 

Eq.  (107) 

315  1  11  123  128.17  1.000  1.00 

311  2  10  116  116.63  0.910  1.10 

282  3  9  109  104.84  0.818  1.22 


286 

4 

8 

93 

93.18 

0.727 

1.38 

319 

5 

7 

85 

81.52 

0.636 

1.57 

303 

6 

6 

65 

69.85 

0.545 

1.83 

321 

7 

5 

51 

58.19 

0.454 

2.20 

317 

8 

4 

46 

46.65 

0.364 

2.75 

290 

9 

3 

35 

34.99 

0.273 

3.67 

288 

10 

2 

26 

23.33 

0.182 

5.50 

301 

11 

1 

20 
769 

11.66 

0.091 

11.00 

2 

2  =  6.001 

=  (N  +  l)/2 

Where  v  represents  relative  velocity  as  calculated  by  Eq.  (108)  for  any 
A^,  empirical  calculation  will  show  that: 

Then  where  wj°^^)   and  7ilf-p^  represent  respectively,   observed    and  ex- 
pected number  of  contacts  in  any  arbitrary  period  of  time,  the  number  of 


150 


John  B.  Calhoun 


contacts,  Hd,  for  any  ith  animal  l)ecomes: 

Vi 


n 


(exp)     _ 


{N  +  1)/2J  t^ 


Zn^ 


bs) 


=    Vi 


2  E  n(f «) 

iV  +  1 


(112) 


And  by  substituting  Eq.  (108)  into  Eq.  (112)  we  obtain  an  equation 
more  convenient  for  calculation: 


„(exp)    _ 
"'ci  ~ 


N 


2  E  nT' 

t=i 

N  +  1 


(113) 


nfl^\  so  calculated,  are  given  in  Table  XV.  Where 


i2=l 


,  (exp) 


X"  =  8.001,  which  with  10  degrees  of  freedom  has  a  p  of  0.629.  On  this 
basis  the  observed  certainly  does  not  deviate  significantly  from  the  expected. 


1.  Awareness  of  Self 

Three-fourths  of  the  contribution  to  the  above  x"  come  from  the  single 
omega,  nth.  ranked  indi^'idual.  Considering  only  the  highest  ten  ranked 
individuals,  x"  =  2.006  which  with  9  degrees  of  freedom  has  a  p  of  0.99!  I 
have  already  shown  (Calhoun,  1956)  that  the  paired  contacts  in  this 
group  diverged  markedly  from  randomness,  and  so  the  divergence  must 
reflect  some  fixed  social  system  such  as  elaborated  here  with  regard  to 
reduction  in  velocity.  Therefore,  the  marked  divergence  of  this  single 
omega  individual  is  likely  to  reflect  a  basic  process,  not  just  a  random 
variation;  so  I  asked,  "How  would  self-awareness  afTect  the  present  formu- 
lation?" By  self-awareness  I  mean  that  an  individual  recognizes  and  im- 
poses self-sanctions  which  are  of  sufficient  intensity  to  reduce  his  velocity 
just  as  much  as  do  the  sanctions  imposed  upon  him  by  his  associates.  For 
this  to  happen  it  means  that  an  individual  can  "meet"  himself. 

Equations  (96),  (97),  (107),  and  (109)  imply  that  an  animal  can  meet 
itself.  That  an  individual  meets  himself  means  that  he  must  recognize 
himself.  This  raises  the  question  of  how  an  individual  recognizes  himself. 
One  way  is  by  comparison.  Considering  degrees  of  difference  depicted  by 
Table  XIII,  an  individual  can  say,  "I  am  at  least  as  different  as  those 


1.   The  Social  Use  of  Space  151 

which  possess  certain  recessive  c?-genes  that  I  possess."  By  this  method  of 
comparison  with  individuals  who  differ  more  than  he  does,  an  individual 
can  know  the  entire  extent  of  his  difference.  This  is  true  for  all  except  the 
7iih.  ranked  omega  individual.  As  may  plainly  be  seen  from  Table  XIII, 
7?ii  has  no  basis  for  comparing  his  d-gene  ^"^j"^  since  no  other  individual 
shares  it.  He  can  only  be  aware  of  his  divergence  including  gi^^---g\^^ 
shared  in  entirety  by  Rw.  Therefore,  by  Eq.  (107)  his 

SM  =  11/2  =  5.50 

andby  Eq.  (108)  his 

V  =  2/11  =  0.182 
Therefore,  by  Eq.  (113)  his 

n^;-p)  =  2.3.33 

Using  this  \'alue  in  Table  XX  the  x"  for  the  entire  group  of  11  mice 
becomes  2.522,  which  with  10  degrees  of  freedom  has  p  =  0.99.  I  realize 
that  this  is  only  one  case  and  I  may  justifiably  be  accused  of  making  a 
"conceptual  mammoth  out  of  a  mouse."  Yet  if  animals  do  recognize  their 
individuality,  the  only  confirmation  of  such  recognition  can  come  through 
observing  that  the  omega  individual  in  an  Nh  group  exhibits  twice  the 
velocity  or  twice  the  number  of  contacts  he  would  otherwise  be  expected 
to  have. 

It  is  realized  that  if  the  iVj  of  the  iVth  ranked  animal,  the  omega  in- 
dividual, is  equivalent  to  that  of  the  (iV  —  l)th  ranked  animal,  the  n^®^^^ 
for  the  omega  animal  will  actually  be  slightly  less  than  23.33.  Likewise 
the  n^^^^^  for  all  higher  ranked  animals  would  be  slightly  less  than  stated 
in  Table  XV,  since 


N  N 

R=l  S=l 


2.  Sanctions  and  Facilitations 

When  the  target  diameters  of  all  members  of  N  are  identical,  then  the 
S,  in  the  sense  of  S'^''\  is  identical  for  all  individuals  and  is  equal  to  1.0.  As 
target  diameters  vary  in  accordance  with  the  system  illustrated  in  Table 
XIII,  S  increases  except  for  the  alpha  first-ranked  individual.  Such  in- 
creases in  S  mean  that  the  intensity  (duration)  of  the  response  evoked 
from  others  will  be  greater.  Therefore,  intensity  of  response,  the  i  of  the 
prior  formulation,  will  increase  by  a  factor  =  aS*"'. 

Let  2^''  represent  the  intensity  of  response  evoked  from  every  member, 


152  John  B.  Calhoun 

e,  of  A^  compatible  with  one's  own  »S'''\  Then  it  follows  from  Eq.  (70)  that 
for  the  iih  individual 


,■(«)     _      C'C'') 


S 


1 


N  -  1 


1/2 

(114) 


{(V)  represents  the  imposed  restraint  or  sanction.  Sanctions  in  this  sense 
lead  to  velocity  reduction  as  exemplified  by  the  mice,  i^"^  =  i'f^  with 
reference  to  self  action. 

Where  the  target  diameters  of  all  individuals  are  identical,  Eq.  (70) 
with  an  AT"  of  11  gives  an  i  =  0.316.  However,  where  target  diameters  differ 
as  given  in  Table  XIII,  it  may  be  seen  from  column  7  in  Table  XV  that 
S''"^  increases  from  1.0  to  5.5.  (Here,  I  am  also  assuming  that  the  A^'th 
ranked  individual  has  the  same  S  as  the  (N  —  l)th  ranked  individual  for 
reasons  discussed  in  Section  XIV,  A,  1.)  The  mean  *S^"^  will  be  2.52.  There 
fore,  the  mean  i^"^  becomes  2.52  X  0.316  =  0.816. 

We  are  considering  intensities  of  interaction  involved  during  imposition 
of  sanctions  in  the  sense  of  restraints.  Therefore,  when  we  consider  any 
individual  in  the  a  responsive  state,  the  other  individuals  from  which  it 
evokes  an  {'-'"'>  sanction-type  interaction  must  be  considered  to  be  in  the  p 
nonresponsive  state  since  the  resulting  interaction  will  not  contribute  to 
this  individual's  satiation  from  social  interaction.  Every  member,  e,  of  A'', 
which  is  in  the  responsive  state,  a,  will  exhibit  an  i^'f  intensity  of  response 
toward  any  ith  individual,  also  in  the  a  responsive  state,  where 


iiV  =  S)'' 


1 


N  -  \ 


(115) 


i^^^  represents  the  intensity  of  interaction  of  the  individual  searching 
for  satisfaction.  Similarly  the  mean  z^^^  will  also  be  0.816  instead  of  the 
0.316  which  exists  when  all  individuals  in  an  A^  of  11  have  the  same  target 
diameter. 


=  0.666 

This  a^j,  represents  the  mean  frustration-type  refractory  period  resulting 
from  the  imposition  of  sanctions  in  an  A^  =  11.  Since  when  all  individuals 
have  the  same  target  diameter,  z^  =  (0.3 1 6)  ^  =  0.1,  hierarchy  formation 
increases  the  average  amount  of  stress  more  than  sixfold.  No  wonder  that 
the  i^gi  elicited  from  others  became  so  eflFective  in  reducing  velocity  so 
that  each  individual's  m  again  equals  1.0.  That  is,  r^S"  =   1.0.  Although 


1.  The  Social  Use  of  Space  153 

intermittent  application  of  sanction,  t^''^  may  be  required  to  reinforce  the 
V  behavior,  sanctions  still  must  be  primarily  a  phenomenon  of  hierarchy 
formation  or  role  assumption. 

When  two  individuals,  x  and  y,  are  both  in  the  a  responsive  state,  their 
resulting  interaction,  ii^H^^^  will  lead  to  a  satisfying  aaa  refractory  period. 
Being  in  the  responsive  state  implies  a  need  for  satisfaction  from  social 
interaction.  Furthermore,  during  such  interactions  each  must  be  focusing 
its  attention  on  the  S^'^^  aspects  of  the  other's  target  diameter.  In  like 
fashion,  when  individual  x  in  the  responsive  state  approaches  2j  in  the  non- 
responsive  state,  X  exhibits  behavior  appropriate  to  ys  *S^^\  while  y 
retaliates  with  a  response  appropriate  to  x's  *S^"\ 

One  individual  which  is  in  the  responsive  phase  will  have  its  behavior 
toward  another  judged  by  the  latter  as  appropriate  or  inappropriate,  de- 
pending upon  whether  or  not  the  individual  approached  is  also  in  the  re- 
sponsive state.  Appropriate  social  behavior  becomes  synonymous  with 
what  I  have  termed  "dominant  (/-genes."  These  behaviors  must  be  ap- 
propriate both  in  form  and  in  timing,  such  that  the  approached  individual 
will  be  in  a  similar  need  state  for  obtaining  satisfaction  from  social  inter- 
action. Even  though  a  social  behavior  may  seem  appropriate  with  regard 
to  form,  if  the  timing  of  its  expression  is  not  harmonious  with  the  refractory 
nonresponse  state  of  the  individual  approached,  then  the  approached  in- 
dividual will  judge  this  behavior  as  being  inappropriate.  Inappropriate- 
ness  in  this  sense  becomes  what  I  term  a  "recessive  c?-gene." 

In  a  perfectly  random  system,  with  all  individuals  exhibiting  identical 
behavior  with  regard  to  its  form,  some  individuals  will  by  chance  more 
frequently  encounter  others  who  are  in  a  refractory  nonresponsive  state. 
Thus,  such  individuals  will  be  responded  to  by  their  associates  in  identical 
fashion  as  they  would  have  been  if  the  form  of  their  behavior  were  actually 
inappropriate.  To  the  degree  that  such  structurally  appropriate  behaviors 
fail  to  be  reinforced  by  similar  behavior  from  associates,  they  may  be  ex- 
pected to  vary  in  some  random  fashion  until  their  form  becomes  relatively 
distinct  from  the  original.  To  the  extent  that  these  alterations  in  behavior 
become  established,  they  represent  clearly  recognizable  recessive  d-genes. 
Due  to  this  modification  of  behavior  S^''^  will  increase  and  S^^'^  decrease, 
so  even  when  the  approached  individual  is  also  in  the  responsive  state  the 
approaching  individual  will  experience  less  satisfaction  from  the  inter- 
action than  it  would  have  had  these  random  processes  not  transpired. 

Once  M  becomes  restabilized  to  1.0  following  the  reductions  in  velocity 
accompanying  hierarchy  formation,  it  will  then  be  possible  for  two  in- 
dividuals, each  in  the  socially  receptive  a  state,  to  encounter  each  other 
and  mutually  contribute  to  each  other's  need  state  by  an  i^^H^^^  -  ««» 
interaction.  In  this  case,  each  individual's  own  i^^^  represents  its  searching 


154  John  B.  Calhoun 

for  social  satiation,  while  the  other's  i'^^  represents  a  social  facilitation,  the 
opposite  of  a  social  sanction. 

I  shall  leave  the  concept  of  social  facilitation  at  this  theoretical  level 
without  seeking  empirical  confirmation.  However,  excellent  data  have  been 
presented  by  Aloreno  (195o;  Moreno  and  Jennings,  1960)  which  permit 
an  exploration  of  the  probable  validity  of  *Sj-'^',  Eq.  (92). 

B.  The  Choosing  of  a  Partner 

We  have  already  seen  that  >S.^"),  Eq.  (107),  and  Si^\  Eq.  (109),  relate 
to  those  response-evoking  capacities  of  an  individual's  target  diameter 
which  determine  the  intensity  with  which  associates  will  respectively  im- 
pose sanctions  or  facilitations.  S\-'^\  Eq.  (92),  and  S^^^  Eq.  (93),  like- 
wise represent  aspects  of  one's  target  diameter  influencing  the  response  of 
others.  One's  own  S'-'^^  determines  the  probability  of  being  chosen  by  others 
as  an  appropriate  object  for  social  response,  while  one's  own  S'^"^  similarly 
determines  the  probability  of  being  rejected.  No  doubt  there  are  excellent 
empirical  data  for  testing  the  validity  of  S'-'^K  However,  I  shall  confine 
myself  to  S'-'^''.  If  I  can  show  the  likelihood  of  aS^^^  being  an  approximation 
of  reality,  it  follows  that  «S^°^  can  be  similarly  justified  as  a  concept. 

Moreno  (1953,  1960)  presents  a  set  of  data  for  which  there  has  been  no 
adequate  formulation  of  their  origin.  In  seven  cottages  each  containing 
exactly  26  delinquent  girls,  he  asked  each  girl  to  choose  three  others  in 
their  own  cottage  whom  they  would  most  like  to  sit  close  to  at  the  dining 
table.  This  instruction  presents  marked  complications  in  determining 
whether  Eq.  (105)  wall  account  for  the  observed  results.  However,  Eq. 
(105)  includes  the  possibility  that  one  wdll  choose  oneself  as  a  partner; 
that  is,  one  will  choose  to  eat  alone.  Moreno  by  his  instructions  excluded 
this  possibility.  Further,  Moreno's  instructions  precluded  the  possibility 
of  choosing  the  same  person  two  or  three  times,  which  Eq.  (105)  permits 
on  successive  independent  choices.  Dr.  Clifford  Patlak  worked  out  for  me 
the  full  set  of  equations  required  to  determine  how  many  times  each  in- 
dividual would  be  chosen,  considering  Moreno's  restrictions,  after  the 
probability,  p,  of  being  chosen  was  calculated  by  Eq.  (105)  for  each  iih 
individual  in  an  N  =  26.  Moreno's  restrictions  so  complicated  the  calcula- 
tions that  it  was  concluded  that  a  simple  lottery  would  adequately  test 
the  applicability  of  the  present  theory,  and  at  the  same  time  avoid  the 
time-consuming  job  of  developing  a  computer  program  to  the  same  end. 
This  was  done  as  follows: 

1.  Sl''^^  was  calculated  by  Eq.  (92)  for  each  of  the  26  members  of  N, 
from  Si'^'>  for  the  alpha-ranked  individual  to  Sii^  for  the  omega- 
ranked  individual. 


1.   The  Social  Use  of  Space  1^5 

2.  Then  the  p  of  being  chosen  was  calculated  for  each  individual  by 
Eq.   (105).  For  example: 


p(-4) 

=  0.152, 

Pi'^ 

=  0.121 

p1^^ 

=  0.092, 

p[V 

=  0.039 

pir 

=  0.012 

3.  We  then  prepared  1000 -pi  tokens  for  each  individual. 

4.  All  these  1014  tokens  were  placed  in  a  large  glass  jar.  For  any  "in- 
dividual" to  make  three  choices,  three  squares  at  random  were 
picked  out.  If  an  "individual"  picked  himself,  the  token  was  returned 
to  the  jar  and  another  "choice"  made  at  random.  Similarly,  if  another 
individual  was  chosen  twice,  one  of  the  two  tokens  was  returned  to 
the  jar  and  another  selection  made  until  each  "individual"  had 
chosen  three  different  "others."  Between  drawings  all  tokens  were 
returned  to  the  jar  and  the  contents  mixed.  Each  such  test  consisted 
of  78  "choices."  At  the  end  of  each  test  it  was  possible  to  tally  how 
many  of  the  26  "individuals"  had  never  been  chosen,  chosen  once, 
chosen  twice,  etc. 

5.  Fourteen  such  independent  tests  were  performed  and  the  mean  number 
of  individuals  in  each  choice  category  was  calculated.  This  mean 
represents  an  estimate  of  the  expected. 

These  data  in  Table  XVI  are  plotted  in  Fig.  36.  A  smoothed  curve  ap- 
proximating the  means  of  the  observed  and  the  lottery  presumably  ap- 
proximates that  conforming  to  Eq.  (105),  considering  the  restrictions  im- 
posed by  Moreno.  This  smoothed  curve  is  called  "theoretical"  in  Fig.  36. 
Each  of  the  two  sets  of  points  varies  so  closely  about  this  curve  as  to  pro- 
vide confirmation  that  my  formulation  is  adequate  to  account  for  the  ob- 
served sociometric  phenomenon  of  choice. 

IMoreno  and  his  associates  make  much  of  the  chains,  triangles,  etc.  of 
reciprocal  or  nonreciprocal  choices  that  became  apparent  in  such  a  socio- 
metric system.  While  such  patterns  may  become  fixed  realities,  there  is  no 
reason  to  attribute  any  condition  other  than  chance  to  their  origin. 

C.  The  Response-Evoking  Capacity  Circuraplex 

Schaefer  (1959,  1961)  presents  a  conceptual  model  capable  of  describing 
the  attitudes,  personality  or  behavior  of  an  individual  in  a  two-dimensional 
space.  This  latter  is  determined  by  two  orthogonal  axes  of  polar  opposites. 
As  may  be  seen  in  Fig.  37,  one  axis  consists  of  the  polar  opposites  love  and 
hostility,  while  the  other  is  represented  by  control  and  autonomy.  About 


156 


John  B.  Calhoun 


Table  XVI 
Empirical  and  Theoretical  Data  Regarding  the  Choosing  of  a  Partner 


Mean  number  of  individuals 

"Theoretical" 
mean  of 

Times  chosen 

A 

B 

Moreno  (1953, 

Present  Lottery 

A  and  B 

1960) 

based  on  S^"^' 

0 

5.00 

5.36 

5.18 

1 

4.15 

5.36 

4.76 

2 

4.29 

3.82 

4.06 

3 

3.72 

2.29 

3.01 

4 

2.29 

2.50 

2.40 

5 

2.00 

1.79 

1.90 

6 

1.14 

1.50 

1.32 

7 

1.43 

0.86 

1.15 

8 

0.86 

0.72 

0.79 

9 

0.57 

0.57 

0.57 

10 

0.14 

0.50 

0.32 

11 

0.43 

0.36 

0.40 

12 

0.00 

0.29 

0.15 

13 

0.00 

0.07 

0.04 

(5 

k 

1 

A 

1 

1 

' 

1 

1     1 

1        {        1        1 
"Theoretical  " 

1        1 

5 

^.^ 

• 
▲ 

Moreno  1953 
Lottery 

— 

CO 

^    4 

• 

x! 

o 

> 

\ 

• 
\ 

^    3 

u. 
o 

CE 

LU 
OD 

i    2 

N 

1 

- 

\ 

1^ 

• 

V5 

^^^^^   • 

— 

n 

1 

1 

1 

1 

1 

1 

1        1 

1    Vl^-i- 

i 

0  5  10 

T  TIMES  CHOSEN 
Fig.  36.    The  choosing  of  a  partner.  Moreno's  empirical  data  are  compared  with  a 
lottery  conforming  both  to  Moreno's  procedure  and  the  probabilities  of  being  chosen  as 
predicted  by  Eq.  (105)  in  the  present  paper. 


1.  The  Social  Use  of  Space 


157 


or  within  the  circle  determined  by  the  extremes  of  these  polar  opposites, 
the  typical  characteristic  of  an  individual  may  be  oriented. 

There  exist  remarkable  similarities  between  Schaefer's  model  and  that 
implied  so  far  by  my  concept  of  response-evoking  capacity,  S,  and  the 
resultant  or  concomitant  change  in  the  behavior  of  the  individual,  such  as 
reflected  by  change  in  velocity. 

*§<'■',   S^^\   S^"^',  and  S^"''  represent  factors  not  specifically  treated  by 


TM.    QUADRANT 

Choleric " 
Ouorrelsomness 
Irritability 
Impulsive 
Aggressiveness 
Delinquent 


I  SJ-     QUADRANT 

Songume " 

Intellectual  efficiency 
Social  participation 
Friendliness 
Leodership 


(HOSTILITY  ) 
REJECTION    (ii)s'°> 


(LOVE) 
ACCEPTANCE 

Ego  strengtit 
Sympathetic 
Trusting 


3  RD.    QUADRANT 

'  Melancholic  " 
Schizophrenio 
Psychosthenia 
Social  withdrawal 


CONTROL 

Introversion 
Repressed  anxiety 
Social  opprehensiveness 
Neuroticism 


2  ND.    QUADRANT 

Phlegmatic" 
Obedient 
Conscientious 


Fig.  37.   The  circumplex  depiction  of  behavior  and  personality  superimposed  upon 
the  coordinates  of  response-evoking  capacity,  S. 


Schaefer.  They  relate  to  his  formulation  as  follows:  They  represent  those 
characteristics  of  the  individual  that  determine  the  probability  of  accept- 
ance or  rejection  as  a  social  object  and  that  determine  the  kind  and  in- 
tensity of  response  elicited.  Schaefer  considers  the  second  logical  step,  the 
evoked  response,  such  as  the  mother's  response  toward  her  children.  He 
also  considers  the  third-order  phenomenon,  that  of  the  personality  de- 
veloped as  a  result  of  being  the  target  of  such  responses. 

I  shall  now  attempt  to  place  these  throe  orders  of  phenomena  in  perspec- 
tive. For  an  A^  =   11,   S^'-'\  S^^\  S^^\  and  S^"^  were  calculated  by  the 


0.091 

1.000 

1.000 

0.000 

0.166 

0.166 

0.244 

0.220 

1.000 

0.000 

0.030 

1.000 

158  John  B.  Calhoun 

above  equations.  These  values  were  then  recalculated  as  proportion  of  the 
maximum  value.  For  examples,  see  tabulation. 


Similarity  rank  S'"'  S^^  S^^'>  S(«) 


1 

6 

11 


/S*"^  and  iS^^^  with  values  of  1.0  form  polar  opposites,  as  likewise  do 
^S^-^^  and  aS^"\  These  polar  opposites  are  shown  as  a  two-dimensional  co- 
ordinate system  in  Fig.  37.  The  four  "response-evoking  capacity"  coordi- 
nate points  for  each  individual  are  connected  by  lines.  Thus,  a  square  may 
be  delineated  for  each  ranked  individual  which  represents  its  "life-space" 
with  regard  to  eliciting  responses  from  associates. 

Schaefer's  two-dimensional  circumplex  description  of  behavior  and 
personality  replaces  the  relative  intensity  values  of  my  axis  of  correlation 
coefficients.  In  most  comprehensive  sets  of  measures  of  behavior  or  per- 
sonality, he  regularly  found  that  two  measures  which  can  be  equated  with 
the  terms  love  (acceptance)  and  hostility  (rejection)  are  highly  negatively 
correlated  and  so  form  polar  opposites  which  may  thus  be  plotted  as  polar 
coordinates  at  180  degrees  from  each  other.  Similarly,  concepts  identified 
by  the  terms  "control"  and  "autonomy"  form  polar  coordinates  opposite 
each  other.  Control  and  autonomy  have  zero  correlation  with  acceptance 
and  rejection  and  so  the  control-autonomy  axis  lies  at  right  angles  to  the 
acceptance-rejection  axis.  From  the  center  zero  point  each  of  the  four  axes 
extend  outward  to  represent  a  maximum  correlation  coefficient  of  1.0  at 
its  extremity.  Every  other  concept  in  the  set  is  then  correlated  with  each 
of  the  four  "key"  concepts.  It  is  regularly  observed  that  every  other  con- 
cept in  the  set  has  positive  correlations  with  two  of  the  neighboring  polar 
concepts,  and  so  each  may  be  plotted  in  this  two-dimensional  behavior- 
personality  field. 

Such  points  approximately  fall  on  the  circumference  of  a  circle  inter- 
secting the  polar  coordinates.  Schafer  calls  such  a  set  of  points  a  circumplex. 
His  schematic  representation  of  types  of  maternal  behavior  is  shown  by 
dots  in  Fig.  37.  Terms  connected  by  arrows  to  these  points  denote  various 
kinds  of  maternal  behavior.  These  maternal  behaviors  are  the  types  I 
would  anticipate  as  being  directed  toward,  and  derived  from,  the  response- 
evoking-capacity  circumplex  determined  by  *S^^\  ^S^^^  S^^^,  and  >S^"\ 

Schaefer  also  reviews  many  studies  by  others  which  harmonize  with  this 
conceptualization  of  behavior  and  personality.  I  have  included  selected 


1.   The  Social  Use  of  Space  159 

terms  in  boxes  which  carry  the  "flavor"  of  what  each  pole  or  each  t^uadrant 
represents. 

It  will  be  noted  that  the  response-evoking-capacity  life-spaces  of  the 
members  of  any  group,  as  I  have  described  their  origin,  mainly  fall  in  the 
first  and  third  quadrant  of  this  two-dimensional  space.  I  shall,  therefore, 
call  these  two  quadrants  the  "primary  life-space."  We  may  inquire  how 
individuals  become  identified  with  the  other  two  quadrants,  the  second  and 
the  fourth,  which  may  be  called  the  secondary  life-space. 

INIy  colleague.  Dr.  Kyle  Barbehenn  provided  the  solution.  In  examining 
Table  XIII  he  noted  its  bilateral  symmetry  with  reference  to  dominant 
and  recessive  c?-genes.  In  any  group  recessive  d-genes  merely  represent 
degrees  of  divergence  from  some  ideal  prototype.  The  alpha-ranked  in- 
dividual possesses  only  dominant  prototype  characteristics,  whereas  the 
omega-ranked  individual,  except  for  commonly  held  (d)  traits,  possesses 
only  divergent  and  therefore  recessive  d-genes.  But  suppose  in  the  history 
of  the  group  some  circumstance  led  the  omega's  associates  to  consider  his 
characteristics  as  being  more  desirable  than  the  alpha's.  Such  a  change  in 
attitude  would  reverse  the  roles  of  all  individuals,  except  for  the  median- 
ranked  individual.  The  individual  who  formerly  was  socially  withdrawn 
and  had  a  low  velocity  would  immediately  become  a  high  velocity  in- 
dividual, participating  in  many  social  interactions.  Likewise,  the  former 
alpha  individual  would  become  the  omega  one,  losing  his  "leadership'' 
role  and  becoming  socially  withdrawn. 

This  transformation  of  recessive  rf-genes  into  dominant  ones,  and  vice 
versa,  will  still  place  most  of  the  individuals  again  within  the  first  and  third 
quadrants  of  the  circumplex  life-space.  The  critical  question  involves  the 
transition  period.  In  this  transition  Eqs.  (92)  and  (93)  change  roles  with 
reference  to  their  application  to  dominant  and  recessive  c?-genes,  as  like- 
wise do  Eqs.  (107)  and  (109).  There  arises  the  hkelihood  that  previous  to 
this  transition  choosing  and  rejecting  will  be  a  more  predominant  activity 
than  controlling  or  granting  autonomy.  If  so,  S'-^''  will  switch  to  pertain 
to  recessive  rf-genes  and  /S^"^  to  dominant  d-genes  while  *S^^^  and  S^"'' 
will  retain  their  orientation  toward  dominant  and  recessive  c?-genes,  re- 
spectively. Or  the  reverse  could  happen  if  the  group  were  primarily  oriented 
toward  the  control-autonomy  axis.  The  consequence  of  these  changes  are 
summarized  in  Table  XVII. 

1.  Shifts  into  the  Second  and  Fourth  Life-Space  Quadrants 
BY  Rats 

No  intent  is  here  implied  of  proving  the  validity  of  the  general  formula- 
tions. All  I  have  hoped  for  is  to  evolve  a  logical  and  reasonable  formula- 
tion that  may  later  pro^'e  to  be  a  fair  first  approximation  of  processes  that 


160 


John  B.  Calhoun 


do  in  fact  exist.  This  elaboration  of  Schaefer's  circumplex  life-space  im- 
mediately brought  recall  of  a  striking  transitory  change  in  behavior  which 
regularly  occurs  in  structured,  dense,  socially  closed  systems  of  domesti- 
cated Norway  rat  populations. 

When  these  populations  have  been  permitted  to  attain  a  density  of  60 
to  100  adults  in  a  space  ideal  for  40  or  fewer  adults,  an  extremely  rigid 
social  structure  develops.  All  rats  "know"  their  place  and  aggressive  ac- 
tions terminating  in  some  individuals  receiving  even  moderate-sized 
wounds  become  markedly  reduced.  Threat  and  avoidance  becomes  the 
predominant  pattern  where  aggression  and  imposition  of  sanctions  di- 

Table  XVII 

Change  of  "Attitude"  toward  d-GENEs  Affecting  Shifts  into  the  Secondary 
Life-Space  of  the  2nd  and  4th  Quadrants 


Initial  change 


cZ-Gene  Interaction  of 

involved  neighboring  poles     Quadrant 


5'"*^  oriented  to  recessive         Recessive        Control-acceptance       2nd 
<i-genes 

S'^"'!  oriented  to  dominant         Dominant       Autonomy-rejection      4th 
d-genes 

S^^^  oriented  to  recessive         Recessive        Autonomy-rejection      4th 
d-genes 

(S'"'  oriented  to  dominant         Dominant       Control-acceptance       2nd 
d  genes 


rected  toward  maintaining  status  roles  is  involved.  And  yet  in  six  of  seven 
such  populations  studied,  one  or  several  males  went  temporarily  berserk. 
Each  such  male  abruptly  began  attacking  all  other  members  of  the  popu- 
lation except  those  that  behaved  as  they  did.  They  inflicted  deep  slashing 
gashes  on  the  bodies  and  tails  of  associates  of  all  ages  and  both  sexes  until 
fresh  blood  could  persistently  be  observed  splattered  about  the  habitat. 

For  any  particular  rat  such  episodes  persisted  from  one  to  several  days 
and  since  usually  several  males  were  involved  the  total  period  of  such  dis- 
turbance might  last  up  to  6  weeks.  In  every  instance  these  bursts  of  males 
going  berserk  followed  a  period  of  at  least  three  months  during  which  the 
investigator  had  removed  all  young  prior  to  weaning.  Then  a  new  genera- 
tion of  young  were  permitted  to  survive.  The  period  of  males  going  berserk 


1.  The  Social  Use  of  Space  161 

coincided  with  the  initial  post-weaning  integration  of  these  young  rats  into 
the  society  when  they  ranged  between  4")  and  90  days  of  age. 

All  the  males  going  berserk  belonged  to  the  high  velocity,  generally 
dominant,  segment  of  the  society,  and  so  belonged  in  the  "sanguine" 
most  desirable  first  quadrant  of  the  life-space  circumplex.  This  meant  that 
dominant  d-genes  predominated  in  their  target  diameter.  The  juvenile 
rats  emerging  into  the  society,  being  less  dilTerentiated,  were  therefore  now 
prototypic  and  therefore  resembled  dominant  adult  males  in  their  posses- 
sion of  mostly  dominant  d-genes.  And  yet  these  juveniles  would  still  possess 
traits  lost  during  maturation  by  most  adults. 

Due  to  the  typical  response  of  rejecting  strange  objects,  the  young  would 
be  rejected  while  still  being  permitted  autonomy  of  action  due  to  domi- 
nant c?-genes  shared  with  high-ranking  adults.  There  being  more  juveniles 
in  the  population  than  adults  belonging  clearly  in  the  first  quadrant  of  the 
life-space  circumplex,  the  general  response  of  rats  to  these  adults  would 
be  to  react  to  them  as  they  did  to  juveniles  by  rejection.  This  rejection 
triggered  the  release  of  the  muted  aggressive  capacities  of  dominant  males 
to  the  extent  that  it  was  expressed  with  great  intensity  even  toward  others, 
such  as  juveniles  and  adult  females,  who  normally  were  not  bitten. 

Such  an  origin  of  an  aberrant  behavior  in  a  rat  society  is  patently  an 
interpretation  lacking  the  complete  documentation  to  carry  the  conviction 
of  its  reality.  Yet  my  intensive  studies  of  rat  societies  permit  identification 
of  so  many  behavior-personality  types  as  to  suggest  that  rats  are  equally 
as  complex  as  humans  in  this  regard.  The  eight  societies  previously  studied 
have  been  commented  upon  in  general  terms  elsewhere  (Calhoun,  1962a,  b) . 

In  the  early  history  of  a  rat  society,  while  its  numbers  and  density  are 
low,  most  individuals  seem  rather  clearly  to  fall  into  quadrants  one  and 
three  of  the  circumplex.  This  is  a  typical  expectation  when  a  straight-line 
hierarchy  develops,  as  it  always  does  in  initial  stages  of  social  organization. 
Later  on,  histories  and  situations  become  more  complex.  Other  types 
develop  which  may  clearly  be  assigned  to  the  second  and  fourth  quadrant. 

For  example,  there  is  the  type  I  call  a  "prober,"  which  appears  to  repre- 
sent a  rat  having  shifted  from  the  third  to  the  fourth  circumplex  quadrant. 
Earlier  in  their  history  they  clearly  belong  within  the  lower  echelons  of  the 
social  hierarchy.  Later  on  they  are  generally  ignored  by  dominants  with 
whom  they  live  most  closely.  In  consequence,  they  develop  a  marked  per- 
sistent state  of  hyperactivity  indicating  autonomy  of  action.  They  seem 
to  generalize  this  autonomy  of  action  as  permitting  them  freedom  of  ac- 
tion anywhere.  Consequently,  they  persist  in  invading  the  domains  of 
territorial  males  whenever  members  of  their  harems  are  in  estrus.  During 
such  invasions  they  rarely  contest  the  status  of  the  territorial  male,  but 
in  the  process  of  being  rejected  by  him  receive  wounds.  These  are  received 


162  John  B.  Calhoun 

so  frequently  that  their  entire  posterior  becomes  a  mass  of  scar  tissue 
de\'oid  of  hair.  Like  other  males  who  still  receive  sufficient  sanctions  from 
associates  to  maintain  their  velocity  at  a  low  level,  they  become  pansexual 
in  the  sense  of  including  adult  males  and  juveniles  of  both  sexes  as  objects 
for  sexual  advances.  They  also  share  with  the  berserk  males  of  (juadrant 
four  the  property  of  heightened  intensity  of  interaction.  Theirs,  however, 
reveals  itself  in  sexual  behavior  toward  adult  females.  Mounts,  instead  of 
lasting  the  usual  1  to  3  seconds,  may  continue  for  several  minutes.  This 
persistence  of  the  mounting,  without  intromission,  resembles  that  of  frogs. 

Low  velocity  male  rats,  those  which  belong  in  circumplex  quadrants 
two  and  three,  fall  mostly  into  two  distinct  categories,  those  which  have 
received  many  wounds  and  those  which  have  received  very  few.  The  former, 
which  belong  in  quadrant  thi-ee,  present  no  conceptual  problem  as  to  their 
origin.  Straightforward  operation  of  the  four  S  factors  will  always  place 
the  lower-ranked  members  of  the  hierarchy  in  the  third  quadrant.  How- 
ever, the  latter,  "phlegmatic"  types  must  have  arisen  from  a  secondary 
180  degree  shift  in  response  evoked  by  the  d-genes.  Their  lack  of  wounds 
during  their  entire  history  indicates  S^'^'>  involves  dominant  d-genes.  Their 
low  velocity  indicates  that  now  as  adults  S^^'>  also  involves  dominant  d- 
genes.  They  are  rats  for  whom  we  may  infer  that  their  associates  have 
always  been  "overindulgent"  and  "overprotective."  Such  rats  are  quite 
fat  and  have  relatively  small  adrenals,  ventricles,  and  kidneys.  Though 
they  exhibit  some  displaced  sexual  behavior,  their  "personality  type"  is 
one  most  characteristically  involving  failure  either  to  elicit  or  initiate  social 
interaction.  They  are  types  lacking  social  involvement. 

This  brief  discourse  on  rat  types  suggests  that  my  elaboration  of 
Schaefer's  (1959,  1961)  circumplex  behavior-personality  complex  may 
prove  to  be  a  fruitful  framework  for  pursuing  studies  in  comparative  social 
psychology. 

D.  Conformity,  Withdrawal,  and  Creativity 

When  N  increases  above  Nb  or  n  increases  above  jU6  it  has  been  shown 
that  fragmentation  of  N  to  appropriately  sized  discrete  subgroups  may  lead 
to  reacquisition  of  6^°^  and  6j°\  Even  when  A^  remains  at  Nb,  the  unavoid- 
able variability  in  the  four  types  of  S  leads  to  yu  differing  among  members 
of  the  group.  Reduction  of  v  as  S'-''^  increases  enables  the  individuals  to 
prevent  excessive  increments  of  6/  above  d'/\  Even  so,  many  individuals 
with  reduced  v  must  experience  either  reduced  da  or  excessive  Of.  We  may 
inquire  as  to  possible  avenues  for  escaping  these  deviations  from  dj^"''  and 
6^/'>  without  leaving  the  group. 


1.   The  Social  Use  of  Space  163 

For  those  individuals  who  in  their  behavior  do  not  differ  markedly  from 
the  alpha-ranked,  Ri,  member,  there  exists  the  possibility  of  adopting  the 
outward  behavior  of  Ri.  Such  acquired  conformity  should  permit  eleva- 
tion of  velocity  to  basic  levels  and  should  produce  a  discontinuity  in  the 
range  of  velocities  observed  among  members  of  the  group.  Whyte  (1956)  in 
his  "The  Organization  Man"  has  emphasized  the  role  of  conformity  as  an 
adjustive  mechanism  accompanying  increases  in  A''. 

At  the  opposite  extreme  of  original  behavioral  divergence  there  exist 
individuals  with  such  reduced  velocity  that  their  contacts  with  higher- 
ranked  individuals  proves  insufficient  to  permit  their  developing  conformity 
through  emulating  the  behavior  of  their  superiors.  Recently  I  have  had  the 
opportunity  of  studying  the  behavior  of  all  members  of  three  populations 
of  domesticated  Norway  rats  for  every  member  of  which  assessments  of 
velocity  had  been  made.  Low-velocity  rats  develop  the  capacity  to  move 
about  without  engaging  in  interactions  with  their  associates.  They  rarely 
initiate  interactions  nor  do  they  elicit  actions  from  associates.  This  social 
withdrawal  becomes  so  complete  that,  despite  being  in  the  presence  of  many 
associates,  they  are  characterized  by  small  adrenals  and  small  ventricles, 
just  as  are  rats  which  have  lived  all  their  lives  as  members  of  A'''s  of  3. 
David  Riesman  et  al.  (1953)  has  movingly  described  such  isolation  and 
oblivion  of  surroundings  among  humans  in  his  "The  Lonely  Crowd." 

In  between  the  velocity  levels  producing  conformity  and  withdrawal, 
there  lies  a  narrow  but  important  range  of  velocity  permitting  a  process 
which  can  terminate  in  creativity.  Why  this  is  so  requires  recall  that  ac- 
cording to  my  model  or  social  interaction,  interaction  with  self  must  be 
included  along  with  interaction  of  self  with  others.  One  can  choose  oneself, 
one  can  reject  oneself,  one  can  facilitate  one's  own  behavior,  or  one  can 
restrain  or  impose  sanctions  on  one's  own  behavior.  All  are  possible. 

Consider  sanctions.  These  are  mostly  the  i['i' ,  Eq.  (114),  of  others 
directed  against  oneself.  Let  i^"'^  represent  self-sanctions.  The  self-inter- 
action becomes: 


N  -  1 


l/2\2 


=  ^   '    ^  (116) 

N  -  1 

The  lil"'^y,  which  initially  solely  represents  self-control  but  may  evolve 
into  creativity,  is  proportional  to  [ij^"'^]-.  As  may  be  seen  from  column  7 
in  Table  XV,  the  intensity  of  this  self-control  mounts  ever  more  rapidly 
as  lower  ranks  in  the  hierarchy  are  approached.  In  so  long  as  self-interac- 


164  John  B.  Calhoun 

tion  only  represents  self-control,  there  can  be  no  creativity.  However,  in 
this  system  what  were  recessi^•e  c?-genes  can  become  considered  as  dominant 
d-genes.  When  this  happens  for  the  whole  group,  a  complete  reversal  of 
the  social  rank  ordering  develops.  But  such  reversal  can  also  take  place 
within  a  single  individual.  In  essence,  this  means  that  [ij-"'^]",  which  is 
equivalent  to  an  a'^p  frustrating  experience,  became  transformed  into  an 
[^i^'^]";  which  is  equivalent  to  an  aaa  satisfying  experience,  without  losing 
any  intensity  in  the  process. 

There  need  not  be  any  outward  manifestation  of  this  transformed  self- 
control.  \_i\^'^^  only  implies  cortical  associations  among  stored  traces  of 
external  events  lacking  any  aura  of  negativism.  Other  than  this,  there  are 
no  limits  to  the  kinds  of  traces  which  may  by  chance  attain  a  high  condi- 
tional probability  of  association.  External  manifestation  of  p,-^'^]",  that 
is  creativity,  implies  sufficient  contact  or  awareness  of  external  events  to 
permit  symbolic  or  behavioristic  alteration  of  the  enivornment  in  harmony 
with  these  heightened  conditional  probabilities  of  association,  [z'i^'^]- 
must  not  be  confused  with  learning.  I  shall  not  attempt  to  go  into  the  ques- 
tion of  learning  here  other  than  to  say  that  it  should  be  most  effective  in 
high  velocity  individuals. 

It  is  well  recognized  that  a  feeling  of  ecstasy,  of  extreme  well  being,  ac- 
companies any  instance  of  creativity.  A  unit  of  ^^°^  arising  from  a  single 
interaction  represents  such  ecstasy.  Recall  that  di°'>  =  0.25.  Examination 
of  Table  XV  reveals  that  not  until  R^.  is  reached  in  the  descent  through  such 
a  hierarchy  of  A^  =  11  individuals  will  li^-'^J  =  IS^/^J/iN-  1)  exceed 
0.025.  It  will  exceed  0.25  for  Rs  •  •  -  Rn  in  an  A^b  =  11.  And  yet  I  have 
already  indicated  that  some  of  these,  probably  at  least  the  last  two,  will 
be  so  withdrawn  from  reality  as  to  preclude  any  opportunity  of  creativity. 
At  most,  we  can  therefore  anticipate  only  2/11  or  18%  of  the  group  to 
possess  potentialities  of  creativity.  As  N  increases,  proportionately  more 
individuals  will  accommodate  by  social  withdrawal  and  relatively  fewer 
will  possess  potentialities  of  creativity. 

Realization  of  these  potentialities  requires  another  set  of  conditions, 
opportunity  for  having  made  many  satisfactory  accommodations  to  new 
configurations  of  stimuli.  As  discussed  in  the  later  Sections  XIV,  G,  1  to 
3,  this  means  that  there  must  have  transpired  an  increase  in  one's  psycho- 
logical area.  A",  as  a  compensation  for  the  reduced  A  associated  with  a 
lowered  v. 

E.  Velocity  and  Home  Range 

In  my  search  for  adequate  formulations  of  the  social  use  of  space  and 
time  I  have  been  guided  by  several  competent  mathematicians.  With 


1.  The  Social  Use  of  Space  165 

regard  to  home  range,  Mr.  James  U.  Casby  (see  Calhoun  and  Casby, 
1958,  pp.  16-17)  derived  a  function,  K/'Iira^-.  He  called  this  term  "visita- 
tion frequency."  It  describes  the  relative  frequency  of  visiting  a  particular 
place  in  the  environment.  Later,  though  published  earlier  (Calhoun,  1957), 
Dr.  Murray  Eden  derived  the  function,  ^l  =  dv/A  to  represent  a  communi- 
cation function  defining  the  relative  probability  of  one  individual  meeting 
another.  K/2Ta~  concerns  arrival  at  a  stationary  point,  while  dv/A  con- 
cerns arrival  at  a  moving  point.  It  was  only  after  I  began  this  elaboration 
of  concepts  concerning  social  use  of  space  that  I  realized  the  isomorphism 
of  these  two  functions.  A  and  2ira^  become  equivalent  expressions  as  like- 
wise do  K  and  dv.  d  here  is  used  in  the  sense  of  S'~''\  Eq.  (107). 

I  have  already  pointed  out  in  the  discussion  pertaining  to  Eqs.  (35), 
(80),  and  (88)  to  (91)  that  when  a  group  is  in  a  steady  state  in  harmony 
with  its  heredity  and  environment,  m  =  1.0,  provided  appropriate  units  of 
time  are  considered.  It  followed  that  v,  S^'^  and  A  also  have  relative 
values  of  1.0  at  Nb,  the  harmonious  steady  state  TV.  Therefore 

t;*S(^V27rc72  =  1.0  (117) 

Given  this  relationship,  can  it  assist  us  in  determining  relative  home  range 
0-?  We  have  already  seen  in  Sections  VI-VIII  that  contractions  and  ex- 
pansions result  from  both  intraspecific  and  interspecific  interactions.  Let 
us  consider  home  range  of  members  of  a  constellation  as  discussed  in  Sec- 
tion VIII,  C. 

On  the  average  the  constellation  consists  of  one  alpha,  6  beta,  and  5 
gamma  individuals,  ranked  in  this  order: 

Nb  =  N.  +  N0^  Ny, 

where  N^  =  1,  N^  =  Q,  Ny  =  o.  The  a  individual  represents  the  ideal 
prototype.  Each  beta  diverges  an  equivalent  amount  from  the  alpha.  Like- 
wise, each  gamma  also  diverges  from  the  alpha,  but  more  so  than  the  beta. 
By  analogy  to  the  discussion  pertaining  to  Table  XIII,  the  rf-genotypes  of 
the  12  individuals  in  a  constellation  will  be  represented  as  shown  in  Table 
XVIII.  as  in  the  table  denotes  alteration  in  the  home  range  a  resulting 
from  the  individual's  S'-''\ 

The  Ni  for  the  alphas,  betas,  and  gammas,  according  to  the  formula- 
tion of  Section  XIII,  B,  5  become  respectively  12,  11,  and  5.  From  Eq. 
(107)  it  follows  that  their  S^''^  are,  respectively,  1.0,  1.091,  and  2.40. 
This  increase  in  response-evoking  capacity,  S^''\  among  beta  and  gamma 
individuals  will  lead  to  their  reduction  of  velocity,  v,  respectively  to  0.9167 
and  0.4167,  as  given  by  Eq.  (108),  when  compared  with  v  =  1.0  for  the 
alpha  member. 


166  John  B.  Calhoun 

In  terms  of  home  range  the  aS'^'^  function  in  Eq.  (117)  may  be  ignored, 
which  means  that: 

l^/27rc72  =   1.0  (118) 

For  this  relationship  to  maintain  in  the  face  of  reduced  v  for  beta  and 
gamma  individuals,  a  must  correspondingly  decrease,  such  that 

a  =  (y/27r)i/2  (119) 

Since  2t  is  a  constant  we  may  ignore  it  for  the  purpose  of  determining 

Table  XVIII 
Relative  Home  Ranges  of  Constellation  Members 

Constellation 

member  d-Genotype  N-,  v  (Eq.  (lOS))       <ts  (Eq.  (120)) 

a  (d)     A    B  12  1.0  1.0 

/3,  (d)     a    B  11  0.9167  0.9575 


/32 

id) 

a 

B 

0.9167 

0.9575 

^3 

id) 

a 

B 

0.9167 

0.9575 

/34 

(d) 

a 

B 

0.9167 

0.9575 

^5 

id) 

a 

B 

0.9167 

0.9575 

Pe 

id) 

a 

B 

0.9167 

0.9575 

71 

id) 

a 

b 

5 

0.4167 

0.6457 

72 

id) 

a 

b 

5 

0.4167 

0.6457 

73 

id) 

a 

b 

5 

0.4167 

0.6457 

74 

(d) 

a 

b 

5 

0.4167 

0.6457 

78 

id) 

a 

b 

5 

0.4167 

0.6457 

home  range  a  relative  to  that  of  the  alpha  individual.  Then 

as  =    (vy  (120) 

In  essence,  this  line  of  reasoning  predicts  that  as  velocity  is  reduced  the 
area  encompassed  by  the  home  range  will  develop  similar  reductions.  Rela- 
tive home  range  sigmas,  derived  by  Eqs.  (107)  and  (120),  as  given  in 
Table  XVIII  formed  the  basis  for  the  relative  sizes  of  home  ranges  within 
a  constellation  (Fig.  27). 

In  considering  the  home  range  of  the  individual  in  the  context  of  mem- 
bership within  a  constellation  of  Nb  individuals,  it  is  obvious  that  the  term 
"area"  possesses  dual  meaning.   (2TrSaa^)  approximately  measures  the  .4 


1.   The  Social  Use  of  Space  167 

of  the  constellation  (Section  VIII,  C),  while  (27r(js^)  represents  that 
portion  of  each  individual's  home  range  within  which  it  spends  0.394  of 
its  time.  Each  individual  simultaneously  "inhabits"  two  life  spaces,  his 
own  and  that  of  the  group  of  which  it  is  a  member. 

For  all  members  but  the  alpha  member,  tiualitative  differences  in  S^'-''' 
lead  to  reductions  in  v,  which  in  turn  bring  about  reductions  in  A  such  that 
the  reduced  A  is  proportional  to  v.  This  accommodation  will  result  in  /x 
again  exceeding  1.0  and  will  thus  expose  the  individual  to  stress  from  an 
increased  frequency  of  contacts.  Further  accommodation,  again  reducing 
M  to  the  relative  value  of  1.0,  can  come  only  through  quantitative  reduction 
of  the  target  diameter,  d,  since  the  S'-"^  qualitative  aspect  has  become  a 
stable  factor.  It  will  be  recalled  that  the  concept  of  the  constellation  pre- 
supposes a  capacity  for  emitting  and  receiving  signals  sufficient  for  any 
member  to  gain  contact  with  any  other  member  despite  their  spatial  separa- 
tion. Such  signals  amount  to  an  increase  in  d  at  the  moment  of  this  emis- 
sion. The  easiest  strategy  for  regaining  a  m  =  1-0  will  be  for  each  individual 
to  reduce  the  frequency  of  signaling  as  much  as  it  had  reduced  its  velocity. 
This  line  of  reasoning  culminates  in  the  conclusion  that  v,  a",  and  d  (in  the 
sense  of  freciuency  of  signaling)  will  all  be  reduced  to  values  inversely 
proportional  to  S^"K  With  respect  to  the  unchanged  relative  values  of  1.0 
for  the  alpha  individual,  these  values  for  beta  members  become  respec- 
tively 0.9167,  (0.9167)-  and  0.9167,  while  for  gamma  members  they  be- 
come respectively  0.4167    (0.4167)2,  and  0.4167. 

Consistent  with  my  objective  of  developing  formulations  adequate  for 
acquiring  data  in  concrete  experimental  studies,  the  above  theoretical 
conclusions  may  be  tested  for  their  application  to  groups  more  compact 
than  represented  by  constellations.  Given  an  experimental  N  =  12  re- 
stricted to  an  area  A,  one  can  estimate  relative  velocity  be  determining 
the  proportions  of  a  series  of  time  samples  in  which  each  individual  is 
active  and  exposed  to  situations  where  social  interactions  do  occur.  This 
velocity  we  can  designate  by  v.  Velocity  so  estimated  will  reflect  true 
velocity  more  accurately  than  the  cruder  measure  of  number  of  contacts, 
Tie,  used  in  the  specific  case  in  the  discussion  pertaining  to  Eq.  (113).  This 
is  because  an  animal  may  be  active  when  all  others  are  at  rest. 

The  more  velocity  is  reduced,  as  predicted  by  Eq.  (108),  the  more  the 
individual  should  restrict  his  travels  to  a  smaller  portion  of  the  area  utilized 
by  the  group.  Of  an  originally  common  home  range  shared  by  members 
of  a  ''compact"  group,  low  velocity  members  will  come  to  utilize  only  a 
portion.  Thus,  the  ideal  design  of  an  area  within  which  social  organization 
is  being  studied  must  provide  for  many  subareas  to  which  visitations  may 
be  recorded.  Otherwise,  reduction  in  home  range  cannot  be  detected. 

Likewise,  the  more  an  individual  reduces  his  velocity,  the  more  he  should 


168  John  B.  Calhoun 

reduce  the  emission  of  signals  eliciting  social  awareness  of  him  by  associ- 
ates. Such  signaling  may  involve  other  than  vocal  modes  and  may  even 
involve  reduction  of  S'-"^  itself.  In  the  end,  such  reductions  of  d  and  S'^"^  will 
produce  an  individual  which,  when  encountered,  is  judged  by  associates  as 
being  dull,  lifeless,  and  with  flat  affect,  lacking  the  attributes  of  an  appro- 
priate object  for  social  interaction. 


F.  Velocity  in  High-Density  Rat  Societies 

While  these  formulations  of  social  dynamics  were  being  developed  I 
was  simultaneously  pursuing  empirical  studies  of  social  dynamics  revealed 
by  large  groups  of  domesticated  Norway  rats.  We  shall  examine  those 
data  which  indicate  (a)  that  densities  greater  than  appropriate  for  Nb 
suppress  velocity,  and  (b)  that  an  increase  in  vitamin  A  above  normal 
levels  buffers  the  social  system  against  the  velocity-suppressing  force  of 
increased  density. 

During  a  16-month  period  of  1960-1961  further  studies  in  the  habitat 
(Fig.  33),  discussed  in  Section  XII,  C,  w^ere  conducted.  In  this  second  series 
of  studies  the  only  habitat  change  involved  altering  the  method  of  providing 
food  and  water.  This  change  precluded  the  development  of  the  behavioral 
sink  discussed  in  Section  XII,  C.  [See  Calhoun  (1962b)  for  a  general  ac- 
count of  these  two  series  of  studies.]  However,  our  concern  here  will  be 
with  results  not  previously  presented. 

During  the  13th,  loth,  and  16th  months  of  this  study  estimates  of 
velocity,  v  were  made  for  each  of  the  32  adult  males  in  each  of  the  two 
societies  considered  here.  All  males  were  fully  mature,  ranging  in  age  from 
10  to  15  months  of  age. 

Procedure  for  velocity  estimation:  As  illustrated  diagrammatically  in 
Fig.  33,  each  of  the  four  pens  in  the  room  defining  area  A  contained  two 
areas  w^here  social  interaction  occurred  most  frequently.  One  was  on  top 
of  the  elevated  artificial  burrows;  the  other  was  on  the  floor  in  the  immediate 
vicinity  of  the  sources  of  food  and  water.  During  each  half  hour  of  observa- 
tion, each  rat  was  given  one  velocity  score  for  each  of  the  eight  locations 
visited.  On  2  days,  not  more  than  3  days  apart,  Dr.  Kyle  R.  Barbehenn 
and  I  each  recorded  such  velocity  scores  for  16  half-hour  periods  during 
each  of  the  three  months  mentioned  above.  For  a  particular  month,  the 
estimated  velocity  thus  consisted  of  the  sum  of  the  velocity  scores  for  32 
half-hour  periods  of  observation.  The  estimated  velocity,  v,  is  here  taken 
as  the  mean  for  three  32  half-hour  sums. 

The  two  societies  were  designated  as  lA  and  2A.  Thirty-two  males  in 
lA  and  32  males  in  2 A  survived  through  the  15th  month.  A  few  males  died 


1.  The  Social  Use  of  Space 


169 


just  before  the  third  set  of  velocity  estimations  due  to  injections  of  a  mon- 
amine  oxidase  inhibitor  (Catron),  which  substance  had  no  influence  on 
the  velocity  of  survivors.  Thus,  for  a  few  individuals,  v  is  based  on  the  mean 
of  only  two  estimates  of  velocity  for  the  13th  and  loth  months. 
■  These  estimates  of  velocity  are  shown  as  data  in  Fig.  38.  Approximate 
regression  lines  were  fitted  by  eye  through  these  points  for  both  lA  and 
2A.  Velocities  of  lA  males  are  markedly  lower  than  those  for  2A  males. 
According  to  the  general  formulation  of  velocity,  one  would  anticipate 
that  males  in  lA  had  been  exposed  to  many  more  social  restraints  or  sanc- 
tions in  the  form  of  aggressive  actions  from  associates.  This,  in  fact,  was 


10  20 

VELOCITY    RANK 


Fig.  38.   Velocity,  velocity-rank  relationships  among  male  rats  in  a  closed  society. 


the  case  as  is  reflected  by  the  amount  of  scar  tissue  developed  over  the 
lumbar-sacral  area  from  wounds  received  in  fighting.  On  the  basis  of  a  5- 
point  rating  scale  (0  =  none,  5  =  most) ,  the  mean  scar  tissue  index  for 
lA  and  2A  males  was,  respectively,  3.24  and  2.49. 

The  lowered  intensity  of  fighting  by  2A  males  was  associated  with  a 
higher  vitamin  A  content  of  their  diet.  Both  lA  and  2 A  rats  were  given  an 
dentical  synthetic  diet  except  for  vitamin  A.  lA  rats  received  3  interna- 
tional units  per  gram  of  diet,  a  high  normal  level  in  comparison  to  natural 
foods.  However,  the  2A  rats  were  given  1 2  international  units  per  gram  of 
diet,  a  level  comparable  to  that  given  to  humans  in  high  potency  vitamin 
pills.  Although  it  is  outside  the  objectives  of  the  present  discourse  to  detail 


170  John  B.  Calhoun 

the  effects  of  increased  levels  of  vitamin  A  on  behavior,  it  suffices  to  note 
that  increases  in  vitamin  A  above  normal  levels  acts  as  a  kind  of  "tran- 
quilizer" which  reduces  fighting  but  increases  the  prevalence  of  abnormal 
behavior  (i.e.,  females  become  poorer  mothers  while  males  increase  the 
frequency  of  exhibiting  inappropriate  sexual  behavior) . 

Previously  we  have  seen  (Section  XIV,  A)  that  in  an  A''  =  11  the  ob- 
served velocities  for  mice,  as  calculated  by  Eq.  (113),  closely  approximated 
the  theoretical.  We  may  ask  the  same  equation  of  the  present  data.  For 
2A  the  sum  of  velocity  indices  [^iLi^i"*"^^]  foi'  IG  hours  of  observation 
was  1089.2,  and  for  lA  it  was  804.7.  Let  v^^''^^  and  v^^^p^  represent  the 
velocities  of  the  alpha,  Ist-ranked,  individual  and  the  omega,  A^th  ranked, 
individual,  as  calculated  by  Eq.  (113).  For  lA,  v^^^p^  =  48.77  and  v^f^'P^  = 
1.52;  while  for  2A,  yj^^p)  =  66.01  and  yjj^^p)  =  2.06. 

Had  all  the  intermediate  v^^^p'>  been  calculated,  as  would  have  been 
represented  by  a  straight  line  connecting  these  extremes  on  Fig.  38,  it  is 
quite  obvious  that  the  observed  w^ould  differ  significantly  from  the  ex- 
pected. One  could  drop  the  inquiry  at  this  point  and  conclude  that  veloci- 
ties for  rats  do  not  accord  with  theory  as  for  the  mice  cited  in  Section 
XIV,  A.  However,  there  are  two  reasons  for  not  dropping  the  inquiry  at 
this  stage.  First,  32  males  represent  an  V  almost  three  times  the  theoretical 
basic  N,  Nb  =  12  while  the  A"  =  11  for  the  mice  closely  approximated  this 
Nb.  Second,  the  regression  curve  of  velocity  for  the  lA  and  2A  males  con- 
verge at  the  omega-ranked  individual.  Furthermore,  this  convergence  is 
at  a  velocity  6  to  8  times  that  anticipated  by  Eq.  (113) .  This  fact  suggests  a 
minimum  velocity,  Vm,  below  which  rats  cannot  reduce  their  velocity  and 
long  survive.  Two  such  individuals  with  excessively  reduced  velocity  are 
shown  on  Fig.  38.  I  can  only  say  that,  on  the  basis  of  the  very  few  individ- 
uals which  did  develop  such  unusually  low  velocities,  such  individuals 
usually  become  bloated  and  usually  shortly  died. 

Accepting  the  indication  of  the  reality  of  Vm,  is  there  a  logical  basis  for 
recognizing  its  relative  value?  In  the  course  of  evolution,  where  the  group 
becomes  adjusted  to  an  Nb,  there  will  in  any  stabilized  group  be  an  Nb- 
ranked  omega  individual.  In  terms  of  Eq.  (107)  it  is  readily  apparent  that, 
where  R  =  rank  in  the  sense  used  in  Table  XIII: 

Ni  =  1  -\-  (N  -  R)  (121) 

Therefore,  for  any  ith.  individual  where  i  is  equivalent  to  its  rank  R,  Kq. 
(108)  becomes 


Z.   The  Social  Use  of  Space 


171 


Since  the  rank,  Ra,  of  the  omega  individual  is  A^;,: 


Va  = 


Nt 


(123) 


Since  Vn  is  the  lowest  velocity  achieved  in  the  normal  Nb  selected  by  evolu- 
tion, Vq  most  likely  also  represents  v,n.  Given  a  v,„  observed,  y£°^^  we  may 
calculate  a  v^J^'p^  appropriate  to  Nb.  Recall  that  from  Eq.  (108)  v^'^^^  in 
relative  terms  =  1.0  and  the  relative  velocity,  v^'^^\  of  any  other  ranked 
individual  is  by  this  equation  represented  as  proportions  of  the  alpha's 
velocity,  v^^^^^  or  v^^^''^  from  Fig.  38  is  12.  Therefore 


12  =  v^'^^^ 


i(exp) 


And  thus  at  Nb,  and  utilizing  Eq.  (123) : 


yCexpat.Vft)     =    ^(obs)  /y  (rel)     —    y(obs)    ^    jy^ 


(124) 


However,  solving  Eq.  (124)  requires  that  Nb  be  known.  The  rats  used  were 
a  domesticated  albino  strain,  Osborne-Mendel.  Nb  still  might  be  12,  as  we 
can  expect  it  to  be  for  the  wild  type,  but  we  have  no  way  of  knowing 
directly  how  domestication  has  altered  Nb.  Furthermore,  the  artificial 
environment  imposed  possible  changes  on  the  area.  A,  factor  in  fj.  =  dv/A. 
So  all  that  can  be  expected  is  that  there  is  some  optimum  A^,  No,  har- 
monious with  the  existing  spatial  structure  of  the  environment  and  any 
changes  arising  through  domestication.  Now,  using  Nb  in  the  sense  of  A'',,, 
values  in  relative  (rel)  terms  become: 

For  N  ^  Nb,  R  =  l:v„  =  1 

For  A^  =  A^6  or  iV  <  or  >  Nb,  R  =  Nb  or  A^:  vq  =  Vm  =  l/Nb 

'N  -  Nb 


For  N  >  Nb,  R  =  I:  vj'^^^  =  1  - 


A^ 


(1  -  v„,) 


(125) 


When  A^  =   oo  :  y„  =  t;^  =  1/A^6 

The  general  equation  for  v,  where  v„   =    1/A^6,  and  R   =   velocity  rank 
becomes: 

_  {1  _  [(jV  -  A^,)/A^](l  -  vj] 
A^  -  1 


,(rel) 


R  -  N 


+  v„,     (126) 


172 


John  B.  Calhoun 


Assuming  the  validity  of  all  assumptions  inherent  in  Eqs.  (124)  to 
( 126)  we  can  now  approximate  the  No  of  the  lA  society  of  rats.  The  regres- 
sion curve  for  lA  shown  in  Fig.  38  gives  v^°^^^  =  38.  Insertion  of  successive 
values  of  Nb  in  Eqs.  (124)  and  (125)  shows  that  when  No  =  9, 
yjexpatA-6)  =  1Q8  and  z'i-^^"  =  0.36.  Since  0.36  X  108  =  38.9,  it  follows 
that,  for  the  strain  of  rat  under  the  existing  environmental  conditions,  9 
individuals  approximate  No  for  male  rats. 

Now  we  may  return  to  the  "tranquilizing"  effect  by  which  vitamin  A 
"buffered"  the  2A  males  from  the  velocity-inhibiting  consequences  of 
A''  >  Nb.  There  needs  to  be  a  correction  factor  in  Eq.  (126)  which,  as  a 
"tranquilizing"  factor,  Z,  increases  the  slope  of  the  velocity — velocity 
rank  curve  also  increases,   "pivoting"  about  Vm.  At  present  there  is  no 


r\ 

IBJ 

\    /V--/^ 

N  Varies 

/    --  0  0 

"^^A^-'i"* 

1         1 

N'48 

I                   1 

I  Z  v-0.5 


Fig.  39.   Hj^jothetical  effect  of  group  size,  A^,  and  tranquilizer,  Z,  on  velocity,  v, 
when  A'^6  =  12. 


a  -priori  basis  for  determining  this  Z  factor,  which  can  draw  upon  empirical 
evidence.  However,  one  can  visualize  a  likely  formulation  of  Z.  Z  implies  a 
factor  "blinding"  awareness  of  the  d  or  ^("^  of  associates.  Such  perceptual 
blinding  conforms  with  the  cV  factor  of  ix'  =  d'v'/A',  previously  alluded  to. 
The  critical  issue  concerns  the  influence  of  Z  upon  r„.  If  the  velocity  of  the 
first-ranked  alpha  individual  never  exceeds  that  appropriate  at  Nb,  that  is, 
if 

:    =    I'M    =     1.0, 


(A' 6)     = 


then  we  would  have  a  partial  basis  for  understanding  how  Z  alters  v. 

In  the  absence  of  adequate  empirical  data  regarding  the  function  of  Z 
on  V,  consideration  of  hypothetical  relationships  (Fig.  39)  will  facilitate 
our  understanding  to  the  point  of  enabling  the  design  of  critical  experi- 


1.  The  Social  Use  of  Space  173 

ments.  First  consider  Eq.  (125).  Increasing  A^  reduces  v  of  all  members. 
By  the  time  N  is  only  a  few  times  Nb,  velocity  of  all  members  will  be  so 
reduced  as  to  bias  the  probability  of  the  social  system  surviving.  Minimal 
velocity  implies  withdrawal  from  social  interaction  and  restriction  of 
activities  to  independent  acciuisition  of  food  and  water. 

Now,  assuming  a  vm,  even  though  it  might  exceed  1.0,  Fig.  39  suggests 
that  increasing  Z  will  eventually  elevate  Va  to  Vm,  while  v^  remains  at  Vm 
as  shown  in  Fig.  38.  Once  Va  reaches  Vm,  further  increase  of  Z  should  reduce 
awareness  of  the  ^^"^  of  others  to  the  point  that  vq  departs  from  Vm  and 
begins  to  approach  vm-  At  Z  =  'x ,  v^  =  Vm.  Far  before  Z  =  cc ,  the  velocity 
of  all  members  of  N  will  be  maximal  for  all  practical  purposes.  A  state  of 
maximum  conformity  will  then  have  been  attained  in  which  each  individual 
views  every  other  one  as  so  like  himself  that  no  individual  imposes  re- 
straints on  the  actions  of  any  associate.  All  social  organization  must  col- 
lapse, leaving  a  state  of  maximally  moving  independent  particles,  the 
random  contact  between  any  two  of  which  will  be  eciually  satisfactory  in 
consummating  any  interaction  in  which  two  individuals  are  necessary. 
This  state  demands  equipotency  of  capacities.  Complexity  of  behaviors  will 
be  limited  to  that  degree  possible  by  every  individual  having  identical 
learning  experience.  Maximizing  Z  becomes  incompatible  with  a  high 
state  of  learning  and  culture. 

The  human  species  appears  to  be  embarked  upon  a  journey  of  both 
maximizing  A^  and  maximizing  Z.  If  we  are  to  avoid  one  of  the  other  of 
the  nirvana-like  states  of  uniform  v^  or  Vm,  it  behooves  us  to  seek  further 
insight  from  experimentation  with  animal  groups. 

Returning  from  theory  to  reality,  we  may  consider  some  correlates  of 
velocity.  Alost  of  the  lA  and  2A  rats  discussed  above  survived  to  autopsy 
during  the  17th  month  of  the  study.  Each  set  of  males  was  divided  into 
five  velocity  class  intervals,  with  as  nearly  as  possible  the  same  number  of 
rats  in  each  velocity  range.  Associated  conditions  are  graphed  in  Fig.  40. 
As  velocity  increases,  the  amount  of  scar  tissue  derived  from  fighting 
increases.  The  somewhat  S-shaped  character  of  this  curve  conforms  with 
historical  events.  In  general,  the  more  rats  withdrew  from  social  interaction 
by  reducing  their  velocity,  the  fewer  wounds  they  received.  However,  a 
few  individuals,  though  having  a  very  low  velocity  in  late  adulthood,  were 
characterized  by  extensive  scar  tissue  because  they  failed  to  withdraw  as 
early  in  life  as  had  their  low  velocity  comrades.  At  the  other  extreme  of 
velocity,  territorial  males  or  highly  dominant  individuals,  who  were 
territorial  in  the  time  dimension  but  not  in  space  (i.e.,  the  "changing-of- 
the-guard"  phenomenon  through  which  several  males  share  the  dominant 
role  in  a  particular  area) ,  by  their  status  avoided  attack  and  thus  avoided 
being  wounded  even  though  they  were  extremely  active  and  inflicted 


174 


John  B.  Calhoun 


wounds  on  associates.  Their  lesser  degree  of  scar  tissue  contributed  to  a 
reduction  in  the  mean  scar  tissue  index  for  high-velocity  males. 

Weights  of  adrenals,  kidneys,  and  heart  all  tend  to  increase  as  velocity 
increases.  Despite  lack  of  histological  studies,  what  I  suspect  has  happened 
is  that  as  velocity  drops  as  animals  withdraw  from  social  interaction,  organ 
size  decreases  in  accordance  with  decreased  demands  made  upon  them. 


20  30  40 

VELOCITY 


Fig.  40.   Some  major  characteristics  of  rats  affected  In-^  conditions  that  determine 
velocity. 


Fig.  40(B)  simply  shows  the  product  of  the  weight  of  these  three  differ- 
ent organs. 

Most  sensitive  to  velocity  and  easy  to  measure  is  fat.  Fig.  40(C).  Fat  here 
represents  those  abdominal  deposits  most  easily  removed:  the  dorsal 
lumbar-sacral  deposit,  that  in  the  genital  mesentery,  and  those  in  the 
mesenteries  of  the  gut.  Though  these  deposits  are  rarely  as  large  among 
males  as  for  females,  it  is  nevertheless  quite  clear  that  as  rats  slow  down 
by  social  withdrawal  they  exhibit  greater  propensities  for  converting  food- 
stuffs into  fat. 


1.   The  Social  Use  of  Space  175 

G.  Exploratory  Behavior 

Ultimate  exposure  to  some  new  configuration  of  stimuli  represents  the 
common  factor  in  the  three  phenomena  encompassed  by  the  term,  "ex- 
ploratory behavior."  These  three  phenomena  are:  (a)  the  rise  and  decline 
of  a  hyperactive  state  following  exposure  to  a  new  configuration  of  stimuli 
(see  Section  III,  A,  4) ;  (b)  the  rejection  of  new  configurations  (see  Sec- 
tion III,  A,  3) ;  and  (c)  the  seeking  of  new  configurations.  We  shall  now 
examine  how  the  opportunity  for  expressing  such  behaviors  alters  an  in- 
dividual's attitude  toward  its  physical  and  social  environment. 

1.  The  Hyperactivity  Phexomenon 

In  an  animal's  normal  habitat  this  phenomenon  may  be  anticipated  to 
follow  an  encounter  with  a  new  configuration  of  stimuli  at  places  in  the 
normal  home  range  where  it  has  not  occurred  during  customary  travels. 
Field  studies  directed  toward  the  elucidation  of  the  consequences  of  such 
encounters  are  essentially  nonexistent.  Pearson  (1960),  by  photographing 
marked  mice  as  they  move  along  their  trailway  systems,  has  found  that, 
following  the  experience  of  being  trapped  and  handled,  mice  not  only  be- 
come more  active  but  also  visit  places  within  or  near  their  normal  home 
range  which  are  normally  infrequently  visited.  My  study  of  the  reaction  of 
domesticated  Norway  rats  following  exposure  to  an  activity  alley  (Sec- 
tion III,  A,  4)  represent  this  same  type  of  situation  with  the  exception  that 
the  induced  state  of  hyperactivity  must  take  place  in  the  presence  of  the 
new  configuration  of  stimuli  represented  by  the  alley. 

Recall  that  the  rat  is  placed  in  a  compartment  with  an  access  door  at 
one  end  of  the  alley.  It  does  not  have  to  enter,  but  many  rats  do  so  rather 
immediately.  For  example  consider  the  73  rats  involved  in  the  analysis  of 
distance  of  termination  of  trips  shown  in  Fig.  3.  Analyses  (Fig.  6)  have 
been  made  of  their  hyperactivity  during  the  initial  2.5  hours  of  their  resi- 
dence in  the  activity  alley.  From  an  initial  high  level,  activity  declines 
exponentially  over  an  approximate  3-4  hour  period  nearly  to  a  base  level 
maintained  on  the  average  through  each  of  the  12  hours  of  normal  height- 
ened activity  during  the  next  3  days.  This  pattern  is  shown  diagram- 
matrically  in  sketch  (1)  of  Fig.  41.  Such  heightened  diffuse  motor  activity 
lacks  any  aura  of  goal  direction  and  will  be  designated  by  the  symbol, 
DMA.  Such  a  configuration  of  new  stimuli  as  is  represented  by  the  alley 
will  be  designated  as  E.  If  a  rat  is  placed  again  in  the  alley  for  2  hours  on 
each  of  several  consecutive  days,  no  appreciable  amount  or  duration  of 
DMA  occurs  on  any  day.  This  means  that  there  has  transpired  an  adjust- 


176 


John  B.  Calhoun 


ment,  A,  to  E  by  the  end  of  the  initial  2  hours  of  exposure  to  E.  After  A, 
motor  activity  persists  on  the  average  at  a  base  line  intensity,  b,  unless 
some  other  new  configuration  E  is  encountered.  Thus,  DMA  represents 
increments  of  activity  above  b. 

In  the  alley,  E  can  only  include  nonsocial  physical  stimuli.  And  yet  in  a 
social  milieu  of  others  of  an  animal's  own  species,  the  responses  of  an  as- 
sociate also  represent  an  E.  Provided  such  a  social  E  has  not  previously 


(1) 

log 
GSS 

Th- 

log 
DMA 

b- 

"_v^ 

(2) 

log 
DMA 

^ 

X 

Tob 

• -Th  Drift 

fa- 

I— 7— "op  4— 

t 

(E,)2 

t 

XE, 

Time 

log 

(3) 

t 

\ 

\ 

t 
No  chonge  in 
threshold 

GSS 

< 

\ 

t} 

aop    ♦          ( 

log 
DMA 

ao"p"'| 

dMA  1 

b- 

Jap  \ 

aop 

* 

1 

i      "■ 

t          A,      t 
E2         2     E 

3 

t 
2E4 

A4 

(E 

\     t 

Eg    EjRemoval 

to  home  cage 


DMA  DECLINE  ONLY  IN 
HOME  CAGE,  THUS  NO 
A    ADJUSTMENT    TO   E3 


Removal 

to  home  cage 


DMA  DECLINE  ONLY  IN 
HOME  CAGE,  THUS  NO 
A  ADJUSTMENT   TO  E4 


Fig.  41.   Adjustment  to  configurations  of  stimuli.  See  text  for  comment. 


been  encountered  or  if  the  associate  is  in  the  nonresponsive,  [p],  refractory 
state,  then  the  latter's  behavior,  though  perhaps  experienced  before, 
amounts  to  a  previously  unencountered  E  in  the  sense  that  it  does  not 
permit  directed  motor  activity  leading  to  a  satisfactory  refractory  period 
by  the  other  individual  involved  in  the  encounter.  For  this  reason,  it  will 
here  be  presumed  that  the  period  of  decline  of  hyperactivity  is  equivalent 
to  the  Gap  frustrating  type  refractory  period  involved  in  normal  social 
intercourse. 


1.  The  Social  Use  of  Space  177 

If  rats  are  maintained  in  isolation  for  several  weeks  or  months  and  then 
placed  in  an  emotional  activity  alley,  a  large  proportion  of  them  never 
go  out  into  the  alley  from  the  "home"  compartment.  From  this  I  infer  that 
there  is  some  upper  threshold,  Th,  of  DMA  which  "overloads"  that  neural 
circuit  permitting  its  expression.  The  three  dots  in  sketch  (2)  denote  such 
overbadings.  In  any  sample  of  subjects,  most  of  whom  avoid  entering  the 
alley,  there  are  a  few  with  various  lengthened  latencies,  causing  delays  of 
up  to  nearly  the  end  of  the  2-hour  test  period  before  initiation  of  the  initial 
phase  of  intense  hyperactivity.  From  this  fact  it  is  apparent  that  the  normal 
DMA  becomes  replaced  by  some  generalized  stress  state,  GSS,  which 
follows  a  similar  (but  lower?)  rate  of  decline  than  the  DMA.  GSS  lacks  a 
striated  muscle  component.  Once  it  has  dechned  to  Th,  GSS  becomes 
transformed  to  DMA.  Such  subjects,  owing  to  their  having  been  protected 
from  impinging  external  stimuli  for  such  a  long  time  during  their  isolation, 
view  the  alley  configuration  as  an  increased  intensity,  E.  An  inference  from 
this  is  that  there  is  a  slow  drift  downward  of  Th  toward  6  during  the  weeks 
of  isolation  when  opportunities  for  adjustments  Ai  •  •  •  An  in  response  to 
El  • '  •  to  En  are  absent.  Thus,  depending  upon  how  extensive  this  drift 
has  been,  any  particular  E,  such  as  represented  by  the  activity  alley,  raises 
the  GSS  different  amounts  above  Th.  I  suspect,  though  my  data  are  not 
conclusive,  that  the  more  elevated  GSS  is  above  Th  the  lower  will  be  its 
rate  of  decline.  These  differences  are  diagrammatically  shown  in  sketch 
(2)  of  Fig.  41.  Given  sufficient  time  with  no  interference  by  other  E's, 
DMA  will  eventually  reach  h  and  an  adjustment,  A,  transpires. 

Now  we  may  consider  the  more  normal  coiu-se  of  maturation,  experience 
with  a  sequence  of  ^i  •  •  •  En  [sketch  (3)  in  Fig.  41].  One  general  observa- 
tion first.  The  greater  has  been  the  experience  of  rats  in  the  sense  of  a  larger 
number  of  different  E's  to  which  adjustments,  A,  have  been  made,  the 
lower  will  be  the  probability  of  withdrawal  in  the  form  of  failing  to  enter 
the  alley  from  the  starting  "home"  compartment.  An  explicit  experiment 
concerning  this  point  has  already  been  cited  with  reference  to  Table  lb. 
Such  results  indicate  an  elevation  of  Th  to  Tha  following  each  A,  and  this 
elevation  will  be  proportional  to  the  magnitude  of  E  provided  E  does  not 
elicit  a  DMA  exceeding  Th.  Thus,  at  some  later  time  an  intense  E,  desig- 
nated as  2Ei  in  sketch  (3),  will  result  in  adjustment  Ai,  although  had  a 
2E  configuration  occurred  earlier  it  would  have  resulted  in  the  undesirable 
consequences  accompanying  elevation  of  DMA  above  Th.  There  is  another 
conclusion,  somewhat  more  tentative,  though  some  of  my  results  do  sup- 
port it.  This  is  that  with  each  successive  E,  the  rate  of  decline  of  DMA 
increases.  In  other  words,  the  aap  refractory  period  decreases.  Thus,  the 
more  adjustments  an  individual  makes,  the  better  he  will  be  al)le  to  curtail 
oif„p  =  df.  I  have  already  mentioned  the  failure  of  a  second  exposure  to  a 


178  John  B.  Calhoun 

particular  E  to  induce  any  material  increase  in  DMA.  This  means  that  if 
E  merely  represents  a  configuration  of  strange  stimuli  with  which  the 
individual  can  interact  in  no  meaningful  way,  the  second  exposure  to  E, 
that  is  (£'1)2,  will  evoke  no  response.  (£"1)2  will  merely  be  ignored.  However, 
if  some  aspects  of  the  Ei  configuration  permit  meaningful  interaction, 
then  (£'1)2  will  result  in  an  interaction  having  an  «„„  refractory  period 
proportional  to  the  evoked  directed  motor  action,  dAIA.  This  a„„  will 
normally  be  of  shorter  duration  than  a„p  to  (£"1)1  since  some  components 
of  the  configuration  are  likely  to  be  irrelevant  to  dAIA.  Furthermore, 
dMA  may  be  expected  to  be  maintained  at  near  its  initial  intensity  until 
the  evoked  behavior  terminates.  No  further  elevation  of  Th  accompanies 
(£1)1  •••  {Ei)n.  Persistent  recurrence  of  any  specific  E  merely  serves  to 
prevent  the  drifting  downward  of  Th. 

Each  adjustment  to  a  new  E  configuration  resulting  in  an  elevation  in 
Th  represents  a  contribution  to  the  individual's  psychological  area.  A" . 

2.  The  Seeking  of  New.  Configurations 

Consider  two  groups  of  individuals,  A  and  B,  of  which  the  members  of 
each  for  a  fairly  long  period  merely  reexperience  particular  sets  of  £"s. 
Members  of  group  A  differ  from  those  of  B  in  that  they  are  exposed  to  a 
larger  assembly  of  different  £"s.  Two  such  groups  have  been  considered  in 
Section  III,  A,  5  and  Table  I  la.  The  fact  that  more  of  the  A-type  individuals 
entered  the  alley  when  exposed  to  this  new  E  configuration  of  stimuli  in- 
dicates that  reexperience  of  a  larger  assembly  of  different  £"s  does  maintain 
Th  at  higher  levels  despite  some  downward  drift.  The  A-type  individuals 
obviously  have  a  larger  psychological  area.  A",  than  the  B-type  ones.  A" 
essentially  connotes  capacity  to  adjust.  In  any  environment  presenting 
frequent  necessity  for  adjustment,  a  high  A"  will  prove  advantageous.  So 
we  need  to  consider  the  question  of  maximizing  A" . 

Persistence  in  repeated  interaction  with  certain  £"s  is  necessary  or  de- 
sirable because  of  acquired  reward  value  accompanying  the  directed  motor 
activity,  dMA,  appropriate  to  these  £"s.  Such  dMA  can  only  preserve  Th 
at  a  given  level.  Response  to  these  £"s  involved  in  this  dMA  consumes  time. 
If  all  w^aking  time  becomes  relegated  to  dMA-evoking  £"s,  the  individual 
will  develop  a  static  A".  Furthermore,  the  more  waking  time  becomes 
filled  with  repetitions  of  response  to  any  given  E,  the  more  restricted  will 
be  A" .  It  is  thus  obvious  that  the  best  strategy  for  maximizing  A"  will  be 
to  reserve  a  portion  of  one's  waking  hours  simply  for  responding  to  new 
£"s.  Some  as  yet  unknown  but  probably  fairly  long  time,  certainly  of  the 
order  of  several  days  even  for  rats,  must  elapse  between  one  exposure  to 
such  a  new  E  and  a  reexposure,  permitting  sufficient  extinction  of  the  A 


1.  The  Social  Use  of  Space  179 

adjustment  so  that  at  the  re-exposure  the  E  eUcits  a  DMA  comparable  to 
that  of  the  initial  exposure.  Because  of  the  limitations  of  time  and  space  in 
which  any  individual's  activities  must  transpire,  maximizing  A"  demands 
an  active  seeking  of  new  £"s.  This  seeking,  I  term  v".  It  represents  a  kind 
of  velocity  difficult  to  distinguish  from  the  normal  velocity,  v,  unless  one 
is  aware  of  the  history  of  an  individual  with  reference  to  its  pattern  of 
repeating  specific  dMA.  In  empirical  experimental  situations  the  relative 
magnitude  of  v'/  may  be  determined  by  observing  the  probability  of  ap- 
proaching a  new  E  introduced  into  an  individual's  home  range  so  that  nor- 
mal movements  will  produce  exposure.  Ecologists  concerned  with  con- 
trolling the  density  of  species  which  damage  human  property  or  serve  as 
hosts  for  diseases  transmittable  to  man  have  been  aware  of  v"  in  a  negative 
sense.  They  (e.g.,  Chitty  and  Southern,  1954)  have  noted  the  avoidance  of 
new  objects,  such  as  poison  baits  or  traps.  This  avoidance  has  been  termed 
"the  strange-object  response." 

3.  Active  Rejection  of  New  Configurations 

Distinct  from  the  relative  attraction  to  or  avoidance  of  a  new  configura- 
tion is  the  phenomenon  of  physically  rejecting  or  psychologically  blocking 
awareness  of  new  £"s.  Processes  included  under  this  phenomenon  of  rejec- 
tion may  be  designated  as  d".  A  grasp  of  the -types  of  phenomena  subsumed 
under  d"  may  be  obtained  through  considering  a  case  observed  in  my 
laboratory. 

Large  "life-space"  cages,  LSC,  were  designed  to  provide  an  optimum 
situation  for  the  breeding  of  the  very  sensitive  wild  Norway  rats  in  the 
laboratory.  A  16  X  25-inch  floor  provided  access  to  an  activity  wheel  on 
one  side  and  a  lever  on  the  other,  which  when  pressed,  provided  a  drop  of 
water.  From  this  floor  two  ramps  led  to  a  partitioned  second  floor  of  eciual 
dimensions.  From  one  side  of  the  second  floor  rats  had  access  to  one  8  X 
8  X  6-inch  nest  box,  while  two  next  boxes  w^ere  accessible  from  the  other 
side  of  the  second  floor.  One  male  and  two  female  adult  rats  lived  in  each 
of  six  cages.  At  the  time  in  question  three  of  these  cages  each  also  contained 
a  recently  weaned  litter.  Up  to  this  time  the  water-providing  lever  apparatus 
had  not  been  delivered  by  the  manufacturer.  In  its  place  the  adults  were 
provided  water  through  a  drinking  tube  from  a  bottle,  as  had  been  the 
practice  since  they  were  captured  in  the  wild  as  juveniles.  When  the  lever 
apparatuses  became  available,  one  was  inserted  into  each  cage  and  the 
water  bottle  was  remo^xd.  By  the  following  morning  when  the  cages  were 
next  examined,  the  situation  in  each  cage  was  identical;  all  movable  ob- 
jects available  to  the  rats,  paper  used  as  nesting  material  and  orange  peels, 
had  been  piled  over  the  lever,  completely  hiding  it. 


180  John  B.  Calhoun 

In  those  cages  containing  recently  weaned  young,  the  young  soon  scat- 
tered the  pile  of  objects,  exposing  the  lever,  and  in  so  doing  accidentally 
pressed  the  lever  and  gradually  learned  its  function.  There  then  followed  a 
repeated  process  of  covering  the  lever  by  the  adults  and  its  removal  by  the 
young.  Through  this  process  the  adults  were  forced  to  face  E,  represented 
by  the  lever  and  its  attached  water  reservoir,  sufficiently  to  permit  an  A 
adjustment  to  E  thi-ough  the  DMA  decline  process.  Several  points  may  be 
deduced  from  these  observations.  Th  for  the  adults  had  previously  drifted 
downward  toward  b  so  that  the  E  lever  configuration  caused  DMA  to 
exceed  it.  Furthermore,  at  weaning  Th  is  sufficiently  removed  from  b  that 
many  ^'s  will  fail  to  evoke  DMA  elevation  above  Th.  Had  evolution  not 
resulted  in  such  a  balance  between  neurology  and  physiology,  animals  just 
emerging  out  into  the  many  E's  of  their  environment  would  immediately 
be  forced  into  a  withdrawal  state.  Although  I  shall  not  go  into  this  problem 
here,  it  is  obvious  that  retardation  of  "weaning"  increases  the  probability 
of  withdrawal . 

In  the  three  cages  lacking  recently  weaned  young,  the  pile  of  material 
covering  the  levers  remained  undisturbed  for  several  days  until  the  rats 
were  so  weak  from  lack  of  water  that  it  was  apparent  that  their  rejection 
of  the  lever  was  so  complete  that  they  would  die  before  getting  the  op- 
portunity to  learn  its  function  through  chance  depression  of  it.  Replacing 
the  former  water  bottle  merely  satisfied  their  thirst  but  failed  to  alter  their 
rejection  of  E.  "Teaching"  the  rats  the  lever  was  finally  accomplished  by 
taking  all  movable  objects  from  the  cage  and  gradually  increasing  the 
interval  during  which  the  water  bottle  was  removed.  It  took  3  weeks  to 
reach  the  same  level  of  lever  pressing  by  these  rats  that  was  obtained  within 
3  days  by  adults  when  young  not  only  made  rejection  impossible  but  also 
set  an  example  of  adjusted  interaction  with  the  E  lever  configuration. 

Other  examples  of  such  d"  active  rejection,  but  toward  social  E's,  have 
already  been  given  in  Section  XIII,  B,  4  in  connection  with  the  three 
examples  involving  Barnett's  Norway  rats,  the  "Freedom  Riders,"  and 
the  formation  of  the  Co7  Colony  IB  of  house  mice.  Thus,  in  terms  of  active 
rejection,  d"  becomes  essentially  synonymous  with  intensity  of  action 
tow^ard  another,  i'-^f  as  given  by  Eq.  (114).  But  d"  must  also  encompass 
the  more  strictly  psychological  phenomena  of  psychological  deafness  or 
psychological  blindness  such  as  characterizes  the  "malingering  type"  of 
individual. 

Note  that  this  consideration  of  exploratory  behavior  has  lead  to  formu- 
lations of  d",  v",  and  A".  As  for  prior  comparable  terms: 

/'  =  (d"v"/A")  (127) 

Here  /x"  represents  a  third  contact  modifying  function  when  relating  to 


1.   The  Social  Use  of  Space  181 

a  social  group.  Just  as  with  /x  aud  n',  so  it  is  apparent  here  that  the  magni- 
tude of  v"  and  A"  will  normally  change  by  comparable  degrees  in  the  same 
direction  and  that  d"  will  approximately  vary  inversely  with  v". 

4.  The  Effect  of  Interval  between  E's,  on  Velocity 

Further  examination  of  the  data  in  Table  lib,  in  the  light  of  the  formu- 
lations relating  to  exploratory  behavior  and  configurations  of  stimuli,  pro- 
vides additional  insight  into  the  origin  of  reductions  in  velocity,  both  v 
and  v".  Refer  to  Section  III,  A,  3  for  other  comments.  Presentation  of  the 
experiment  in  terms  of  the  present  formulations  is  as  follows : 

The  subjects  consisted  of  male  albino  Osborne-Mendel  strain  rats  iso- 
lated at  weaning.  At  this  time  each  rat  was  placed  in  a  6  X  6  X  8  inch 
cage  from  which  it  could  not  see  out.  Water  and  food  were  introduced 
through  channels  from  the  outside  which  prevented  the  rats  from  seeing 
the  experimenters  or  being  handled  by  them.  AH  rats  remained  in  such 
isolation  for  approximately  three  months  before  further  treatment.  These 
isolation  cages  may  be  termed  an  Ei  configuration  of  stimuh.  Due  to  the 
long  absence  of  opportunity  for  adjustment  to  new  configurations  of 
stimuli,  the  Th  of  all  rats  probably  drifted  toward  b. 

At  the  end  of  the  three  months  of  isolation  the  subjects  were  divided 
into  four  groups:  A  (20  rats),  B  (24  rats),  C  (16  rats),  and  D  (16  rats).  On 
each  of  10  days  during  the  next  two  weeks  each  member  of  Group  C  was 
exposed  for  two  hours  to  a  new  configuration  of  stimuli,  E2,  which  was  a 
Wahman  activity  wheel;  all  rats  so  exposed  entered  the  wheel  and  ran 
during  each  of  the  10  days.  Similarly,  members  of  Group  C  were  exposed 
to  an  E3  configuration  of  stimuh.  This  exposure  consisted  of  placing  the 
rats  in  a  2  X  o-foot  pen  where  they  had  the  opportunity  to  climb  onto  a 
central  platform  where  a  lever  could  be  pressed  to  receive  a  drop  of  water. 
Each  day  of  this  10-day  training  period  half  the  members  of  Group  D 
were  exposed  for  two  hours  to  E2  and  then,  immediately  following,  for 
two  hours  to  Ez.  The  other  half  of  Group  D  were  similarly  treated  but  were 
exposed  to  E3  just  prior  to  E2.  Group  A  remained  in  their  isolation  cages 
during  these  two  weeks. 

During  the  third  experimental  week  every  rat  in  all  four  groups  was  given 
a  two-hour  exposure  to  the  NIH  Emotional  Activity  Alley  on  each  of 
four  successive  days.  This  alley  represented  an  E4  configuration  of  stimuli. 
For  half  the  rats  in  each  group  Ei  had  a  stationary  floor,  a  condition  we 
may  designate  as  Ei^.  For  the  remaining  rats  Ei  had  a  tilting  floor  which 
clanged  as  the  rats  ran  across  it.  This  modification  is  designated  as  Eis, 
which  represents  a  much  more  intense  or  strange  configuration  than  EiA. 
Many  rats  avoided  entering  the  alley. 


182  John  B.  Calhoun 

Regardless  of  the  amount  of  prior  opportunity  to  adjust  to  novel  con- 
figurations, the  more  intense  £"48  elicited  a  more  marked  avoidance  than 
did  Ei\.  (See  Section  III,  A,  3.)  However,  our  present  concern  is  with 
a  different  aspect  of  the  results  in  Table  lib. 

I'pon  exposure  to  EiA  it  appears  that  prior  experiences  with  E^  was 
much  more  effecti\'e  than  with  E-i  in  reducing  avoidance  of  the  EiA  con- 
figuration. But  members  of  both  Groups  B  and  C  evinced  much  less 
avoidance  of  Ei  than  did  members  of  Clroup  A.  This  supports  the  formula- 
tion that  prior  opportunity  to  adjust  to  new  configurations  elevates  Th 
so  that  at  a  following  exposure  to  another  new  configuration  of  stimuli, 
DMA  is  less  likely  to  exceed  Th.  Avoiding  entering  the  alley  is  taken  as 
evidence  of  DMA  exceeding  Th. 

These  results  confirmed  prior  hypotheses.  However,  it  was  further  as- 
sumed that  rats  of  Group  D  would  exhibit  the  most  marked  accommoda- 
tion to  Ei  since  they  would  ha^-e  had  twice  the  opportunity  for  making 
adjustments  to  new  E's.  And  yet  e\ei\  to  E^a,  the  rats  of  Group  D  showed 
little  better  capacity  for  adjustment  than  did  members  of  Group  B,  and 
much  less  than  did  rats  of  Group  C.  Upon  elevation  of  the  intensity  of 
Ei  to  EiB,  members  of  Group  D  exhibited  an  extremely  more  marked  reduc- 
tion in  capacity  to  adjust  than  did  the  rats  of  Groups  B  and  C,  that  pre- 
sumably had  less  opportunity  for  "training"  in  making  adjustments. 

These  results  apparently  contradict  the  theory.  But  consider  the  follow- 
ing. For  rats  exposed  to  new  configurations  of  stimuli,  such  as  £"4,  but 
permitted  to  remain  for  several  rather  than  for  2  hours,  it  has  been  noted 
that  many  individuals  reciuire  up  to  3  hours  for  DMA  to  decline  to  h.  In 
the  2000-odd  tests  where  rats  have  been  exposed  to  the  AVtype  alley  con- 
figuration to  test  for  emotionality,  the  tacit  assumption  has  been  made 
that  the  remaining  decline  in  DMA  will  take  place  after  the  return  of  the 
rats  to  their  accustomed  environment.  However,  this  opportunity  did  not 
prevail  when,  after  2  hours  in  E2  or  E^,  rats  were  transferred  to  the 
opposite  E. 

Events  presumably  transpiring  are  diagrammed  in  the  left-hand  side  of 
sketch  (4)  in  Fig.  41.  Upon  exposure  to  Es  after  only  partial  decline  of 
DMA  following  exposure  to  E2,  the  same  increment  in  DMA  is  elicited, 
but  its  rise  starts  at  the  point  of  a  still  fairly  high  level  of  DAIA.  Thus, 
this  second  increment  in  DMA  forces  it  above  the  threshold,  Th,  where 
DjVIA  is  transformed  from  difi'use  motor  activity  into  the  generalized  stress 
state,  GSS.  Although  GSS  and  DMA  were  not  measured  during  E3  (or 
during  E2,  if  it  came  second  for  Group  D) ,  both  must  have  eventually  com- 
pletely disappeared  after  the  usual  return  to  the  home  cage.  And  yet  the 
very  failure  of  many  members  of  Group  D  to  enter  the  Ei  alley  when  given 


1.   The  Social  Use  of  Space 


183 


exposure  to  it  suggests  that  for  each  degree  of  GSS  induced  by  too  close 
spacing  of  consecutive  new  E's,  there  had  transpired  a  drop  in  Th  and  B 
such  that  Th  drops  relatively  more  than  h.  Each  of  the  ten  opportunities  for 
consecutive  exposure  to  E2  and  Es  must  have  narrowed  the  gap  between  h 
and  Th.  Thus  the  later  exposure  to  Ei  must  have  caused  "overloading" 
for  most  Group  D  rats  to  the  extent  that  GSS  had  not  declined  to  Th  by 
the  end  of  the  two  hours  in  Ei.  This  meant  that  decline  in  DMA  to  base 
level  took  place  in  Ei.  Return  to  Ei  for  2  hours  on  each  of  the  following  3 
days  was  characterized  by  persistence  in  avoiding  entry  into  the  Ei  alley 
configuration  by  most  rats  avoiding  it  on  first  exposure.  Avoidance,  v",  of 
any  new  E  will  thus  be  proportional  to  the  degree  of  GSS  "overloading" 
elicited  by  the  new  E's. 

Although  new  E's  have  been  considered  above  in  the  sense  of  physical 
nonsocial  configurations,  we  may  consider  the  consequences  of  too  closely 
spaced  new  or  undesirable  £"s  in  the  social  sense  of  i^'^'s  of  associates.  Re- 
call that  such  i^^^'s  represent  social  restraints  or  sanctions  imposed  by 
associates.  For  such  sanctions  to  become  effective  in  reducing  velocity,  v, 
that  is  for  causing  a  drop  in  baseline  of  activity,  consecutive  sanctions  by 
the  same  or  different  associates  must  be  sufficiently  closely  spaced  to  induce 
a  GSS.  No  opportunity  for  v"  avoidance  is  possible.  In  fact,  with  the  drop 
in  velocity  v  (synonymous  with  h),  psychological  area  A"  must  be  re- 
stricted through  failure  to  make  adjustments  A  to  E  while  still  in  the 
presence  of  E.  And  as  we  have  seen,  as  A"  decHnes  so  will  v".  This  means 
that  as  velocity,  v,  declines,  the  individuals  have  even  less  capacity  to 
avoid  strange  stimuli.  However,  recall  that  as  v"  declines  d"  increases. 

I  can  cite  no  quantitative  data  to  support  this  conclusion  of  d"  increasing 
as  y"  decreases.  However,  the  following  observations  support  its  reality. 
While  making  the  observations  on  velocity  of  rats  summarized  in  Fig.  38,  I 
was  consistently  impressed  by  the  manner  in  which  most  very  low-velocity 
rats  moved  "through"  their  associates.  During  those  rare  times  when 
active,  they  would  pass  by  associates  as  if  completely  psychologically 
blind  to  their  presence.  Furthermore,  their  blase,  unaffective  mode  of 
posture  equally  failed  to  elicit  response  from  associates.  The  completeness 
of  this  psychological  rejection  of  reality  is  reflected  in  their  smaller  organ 
weight  and  larger  amount  of  fat  (Fig.  40),  corresponding  to  states  char- 
acterizing rats  maintained  in  approximate  isolation  by  restrictions  to 
groups  of  1  male  with  2  females  in  small  cages  or  pens. 

A  further  corroborative  observation  comes  from  the  study  referred  to 
in  Section  XH,  A.  Among  the  wild  Norway  rats  in  that  study  was  a  small 
group  designated  as  possessing  an  array  of  aberrant  symptoms  and  be- 
havior which  I  called  the  "syndrome  of  the  social  outcast."  From  compari- 


184  John  B.  Calhoun 

son  with  rats  described  in  Sections  XII,  C  and  XIV,  F,  I  now  know  that 
these  social  outcasts  must  have  been  very  low-velocity  rats.  Of  all  the  rats 
involved  in  the  study  referred  to  in  Section  XII,  A,  they  were  the  only 
ones  ever  caught  in  "Havahart"  traps.  These  large,  shiny  wire-meshed 
traps  with  a  door  opened  at  either  end  and  shiny  metal  treadles  on  the 
center  floor  were  regularly  placed  on  trials.  All  other  rats  invariably  ran 
around  these  traps.  Yet  the  social  outcasts  apparently  ran  into  them  with- 
out ever  sensing  their  presence.  This  sensory  unawareness  is  the  low  v" 
factor  characterizing  low-velocity  rats. 


XV.  Conclusion 

Man  did  emerge  from  the  trials,  successfully  overcome,  of  a  myriad  of 
ever  more  simple  forms.  I  have  attempted  to  formulate  some  phenomena 
which  have  affected  man's  social  evolution.  Some  of  these  phenomena 
appear  no  longer  directly  operati^'e  on  the  human  animal.  They  neverthe- 
less left  their  imprint  on  man's  capacity  to  adjust  physiologically  and 
psychologically  to  the  social  system  in  which  he  lives.  Foremost  among  these 
presumed  legacies  are  the  limitations  imposed  upon  him  from  his  origin 
out  of  an  evolutionary  line  in  which  optimum  adjustment  demanded  living 
in  small  groups,  not  exceeding  twice  twelve  individuals. 

Only  yesterday,  as  one  may  measure  evolutionary  time  in  units  of  ten 
thousand  years,  did  man  begin  his  attempt  to  escape  this  evolutionary 
bond.  But  cultural  evolution  has  not  produced  escape  from  this  bond, 
merely  accommodation  to  it.  Human  society  has  developed  the  form  of  a 
many-layered  chain  link  armor.  Each  link  is  composed  of  not  much  less 
than,  nor  many  more  than,  twelve  individuals.  The  links  have  a  fluid 
character.  Through  time,  any  one  individual  shifts  his  membership  back 
and  forth  among  several  joining  links.  This  poetic  view  embodies  the  es- 
sence of  reality. 

In  contrast  to  such  evolutionary  legacies,  there  exist  certain  principles 
of  social  physics  which  must  affect  all  social  animals,  man  included.  These 
principles  derive  from  certain  universals  I  have  called  velocity,  target 
diameter,  area,  basic  group  size,  the  social  refractory  period,  threshold  for 
tolerance  for  change,  and  the  like.  I  am  fully  cognizant  that  my  formulations 
represent  only  crude  approximations  to  reality,  and  may  in  fact  contain 
several  errors  of  logic.  Yet  we  must  develop  adequate  formulations  along 
the  lines  I  have  attempted  or  all  efforts  to  gain  insight  into  the  individual's 
involvement  in  social  action  will  prove  sterile. 

This  sterility  will  crown  the  endeavors  of  both  classical  physiology  and 


7.   The  Social  Use  of  Space  185 

psychology.  We  can  no  longer  afford  to  ignore  the  impact  of  the  social 
setting  on  the  individual's  behavior  and  physiology.  And  without  knowl- 
edge of  evolutionary  limitations  and  universal  principles  of  social  physics, 
consideration  of  the  social  setting  will  also  prove  of  little  avail.  The  search 
for  conceptualizations,  adequate  for  furthering  this  objective,  serves  as  the 
justification  for  inclusion  of  my  effort  to  introduce  the  more  strictly  physio- 
logical discussions  by  the  other  authors  in  these  volumes. 


References 

Bamett,  S.  A.  (1955).  Competition  among  wild  rats.  Nature  175, 126. 

Birdsell,  J.  B.  (1953).  Some  enivronmental  and  cultural  factors  influencing  the  structur- 
ing of  Australian  Aboriginal  populations.  Am.  Naturalist  87,  169-207. 
Birdsell,  J.  B.   (1957).  Some  population  problems  involving  Pleistocene  man.  Symp 

on  Quant.  Biol.  22,  47-69. 
Braidwood,  R.  J.,  and  Reed,  C.  A.  (1957).  The  achievement  and  early  consequences  of 

food-production.  Symp.  on  Quant.  Biol.  22,  19-31. 
Broadbent,  D.  E.  (1958).  "Perception  and  Communication."  Pergamon  Press,  New  York. 
Caldwell,  J.  M.,  Ranson,  S.  W.,  and  Sacks,  J.  G.  (1951).  Group  panic  and  other  mass 

disruptive  reactions.  U.S.  Armed  Forces  Med.  J.  2,  541-567. 
Calhoun,  J.  B.  (1949).  A  method  of  self-control  of  population  growth  among  mammals 

living  in  the  wild.  Science  109,  333-335. 
Calhoun,  J.  B.  (Ed.)  (1949-1957).  Annual  reports— North  American  Census  of  Small 

Mammals. 
Calhoun,  J.  B.  (1952).  The  social  aspects  of  population  dynamics.  J.  Mammal.  33,  139- 

159. 
Calhoun,  J.  B.  (1956).  A  comparative  study  of  the  social  behavior  of  two  inbred  strains 

of  house  mice.  Ecol.  Monographs  26,  81-103. 
Calhoun,  J.  B.  (1957).  Social  welfare  as  a  variable  in  population  dynamics.  Cold  Spring 

Harbor  Symp.  on  Quant.  Biol.,  22:339-956. 
Calhoun,  J.  B.  (1962a).  A  behavioral  sink.  In  "Roots  of  Behavior"  (E.  L.  Bliss,  ed.). 

Chapter  22.  Harper  Hoeber,  New  York. 
Calhoun,  J.   B.    (1962b).   Population  density  and  social  pathology.    Sci.    Am.    206, 

139-146. 
Calhoun,  J.  B.  (1963).  The  ecology  and  sociology  of  the  Norway  rat.  U.  S.  Public  Health 

Service  Pub.  No.  1008,  Government  Printing  Office.  288  pp. 
Calhoun,  J.  B.,  and  Casby,  J.  U.  (1958).  Calculation  of  home  range  and  density  of  small 

mammals.  U.  S.  Public  Health  Monograph  No.  55. 
Calhoun,  J.  B.,  and  Webb,  W.  L.   (1953).    Induced  emigrations  of  small  mammals. 

Science  117,  358-360. 
Carpenter,  C.  R.  (1962j.  Field  studies  of  a  primate  population.  In  "Roots  of  Behavior" 

(E.  L.  Bliss,  ed.).  Chapter  21,  Harper  (Hoeber),  New  York. 
Chitty,  D.,  and  Southern,  H.  N.  (1954).  "Control  of  Rats  and  Mice."  Oxford  Univ. 

Press  (Clarendon),  London  and  New  York. 
Dahlberg,  B.  L.,  and  Guettinger,  R.  C.  (1956).  The  white-tailed  deer  in  Wisconsin. 

Wisconsin  Conservation  Dept.  Tech.  Wildlife  Bull.  No.  14. 
Deevey,  E.  S.,  Jr.  (1960).  The  human  population.  Sci.  Am.  203:194-204. 


186  John  B.  Calhoun 

Duff,  W.,  and  Kew,  M.  (1957).  Anthony  Island,  a  Home  of  the  Haidas.  Report  of  the 

Provincial  Museum  of  Natural  History  and  Anthropology. 
Elton,  C.  (1942).  "Voles,  Mice  and  Lemmings — Problems  in  Population  Dynamics." 

Oxford  Univ.  Press,  London  and  New  York. 
Fredericson,  E.  (1951).  Time  and  aggression.  Psychol.  Rev.  58,  41-51. 
Hall,  C.  S.  (1934).  Defecation  and  urination  as  measure  of  individual  differences  in 

emotionalit}"  in  the  rat.  Psychol.  Bull.  31,  604. 
Hallowell,  A.  L  (1960).  Self,  society,  and  culture  in  phylogenetic  perspective.  In  "The 

Evolution  of  Man."  (S.  Tax,  ed.),  pp.  309-372.  Univ.  of  Chicago  Press,  Chicago, 

Illinois. 
Hardin,  G.  (1960).  The  competitive  exclusion  principle.  Science  131,  1292-1297. 
Hayne,  D.  W.  (1949).  Two  methods  for  estimating  population  from  trapping  records. 

/.  Mammal.  30,  399-411. 
Hutchinson,  G.  E.  (1957).  Concluding  remarks  (to  Population  Studies:  Animal  Ecology 

and  Demography)  Cold  Spring  Harbor  Symposia  on  Quant.  Biol.  22,  415-427. 
Kabat,  C,  CoUias,  N.  E.,  and  Guettinger,  R.  C.  (1953).  Some  winter  habits  of  white- 
tailed  deer  and  the  development  of  census  methods  in  the  Flag  yard  of  northern 

Wisconsin.  Wisconsin  Conservation  Dept.  Tech.  Wildlife  Bull.  No.  7. 
Leopold,  A.  S.,  Rine.y,  T.,  McCain,  R.,  and  Tevis,  L.,  Jr.  (1951).  The  jawbone  deer  herd. 

Slate  of  Calif.  Dept.  Nat.  Resources,  Game  Bull.  No  4. 
Moreno,  J.  L.  (1953).  "Who  Shall  Survive?"  Beacon  House,  Beacon,  New  York. 
Moreno,  J.  L.,  and  Jennings,  H.  H.  (1960).  Statistics  of  social  configuration,  pp.  19-51. 

In  "The  Sociometry  Reader"  (J.  L.  Moreno  et  ah,  eds.).  The  Free  Press,  Glencoe, 

Illinois. 
Patric,  E.  F.  (1958).  Some  Properties  of  the  Small  Mammal  Populations  of  the  Hunting- 
ton Wildlife  Forest.  Ph.D.  Thesis,  College  of  Forestry,  New  York  State  Univ., 

Syracuse,  New  York. 
Pearson,  O.  P.  (1960).  Habits  of  Microtus  calif ornicus  revealed  by  automatic  photo- 
graphic recorders.  Ecol.  Monographs  30,  232-249. 
Ranson,  S.  W.  (1949).  The  normal  battle  reaction:  Its  relation  to  the  pathologic  battle 

reaction.  In  "Combat  Psychiatry,"  Bull.  U.S.  Army  Med.  Dept.  9,  SuppL,  3-11. 
Riesman,  D.,  Glazer,  N.,  and  Denney,  R.  (1953).  "The  Lonely  Crowd."  Doubleday, 

Garden  City,  New  York. 
Sanders,  R.  D.  (1939).  Results  of  a  study  of  the  harvesting  of  white-tailed  deer  in  the 

Chequamegon  National  Forest.  Trans.  North  A)n.  Wildlife  Conf.  4,  549-553. 
Schaefer,  E.  S.  (1959).  A  circumplex  model  for  maternal  behavior.  J.  Abnormal  Social 

Psychol.  59,  226-235. 
Schaefer,  E.  S.  (1961).  Converging  conceptual  models  for  maternal  behavior  and  for 

child  behavior,  pp.  124-146.  In  "Parental  Attitudes  and  Child  Behavior"  (J.  C. 

Glidewell,  ed.),  C.  C.  Thomas,  Springfield,  Illinois. 
Schneirla,  T.  C,  and  Tobach,  E.  (1962).  Eliminative  responses  in  mice  and  rats  and  the 

problem  of  "emotionahty."  In  "Roots  of  Behavior"  (E.  L.  Bliss,  ed.).  Chapter  15. 

Harper  (Hoeber),  New  York. 
Schorger,  A.  W.  (1953).  The  white-tailed  deer  in  early  Wisconsin.  Trans.  Wisconsin 

Acad.  Sci.,  Arts  and  Letters  42,  197-247. 
Shiras,  G.  III.  (1921).  The  wildlife  of  Lake  Superior,  past  and  present.  Natl.  Geograph. 

Mag.  40(2),  113-204. 
Stewart,  J.  Q.  (1948).  Concerning  social  physics.  Sci.  American  178,  20-23. 
Swift,  E.  (1948).  Wisconsin's  deer  damage  to  forest  reproduction  survey — final  report. 
Wisconsin  Conservation  Dept.  Publ.  347. 


1.   The  Social  Use  of  Space  187 

Swift,  E.  (1946).  A  history  of  Wisconsin  deer.  Wisconsin  Conservation  Dept.  Publ.  323. 

von  Foerster,  H.,  Mora,  P.  M.,  and  Amiot,  L.  W.  (1960).  Doomdsay:  Friday,  13  Novem- 
ber, A.  D.  2026.  Science  132,  1291-1295. 

Webb,  W.  L.,  and  Rosasco,  E.  M.  (1953).  An  Investigation  of  the  Properties  of  Induced 
Mass  Movements  of  the  Small  Mammals  of  the  Genera,  Peromijscus,  Clethrionomys, 
Blarina  and  Sorex.  100  pp.  mimeographed.  College  of  Forestry,  State  University  of 
New  York.  Re  Contract  DA-49-007-MD-325,  Medical  Research  and  Development 
Board,  Office  of  the  Surgeon  General,  U.  S.  Army. 

Whyte,  W.  H.  (1956).  "The  Organization  Man."  Simon  and  Schuster,  New  York. 

Zimmerman,  C.  C,  and  Broderick,  C.  B.  (1954).  Nature  and  role  of  informal  family 
groups.  Marriage  and  Family  Living  16,  107-111. 

Zimmerman,  C.  C,  and  Cervantes,  L.  F.  (1960).  "Successful  American  Families." 
Pageant  Press,  New  York. 

Zippin,  C.  (1956).  An  evaluation  of  the  removal  method  of  estimating  animal  populations. 
Biometrics  12,  163-189. 


Endocrine  Adaptive  Mechanisms 
and  the  Physiologic  Regulation 
of  Population  Growth* 


J.  J.  CHRISTIAN  V^^ 

Division  of  Endocrinology  and  Reproduction,  Research  Laboratories, 
Albert  Einstein  Medical  Center  {North),  Philadelphia,  Pennsylvania 

General  Introduction 189 

Part  1.  The  Endocrine  Adaptive  Mechanisms 191 

I.  Introduction 191 

II.  The  Endocrine  Glands  of  Adaptation 192 

A.  The  Adrenal  Glands 192 

B.  The  Thyroid  Gland 228 

C.  Other  Endocrine  Adaptive  Factors 240 

D.  General  Measurements  of  the  Endocrine  Adaptive  Responses 242 

Part  2.  Physiologic  Adaptation  and  Mammalian  Populations 261 

I.  Introduction 261 

II.  Endocrine  Responses  to  Social  Pressures  and  to  Population  Density-  -  263 

A.  Experiments  in  the  Laboratory  with  Populations  of  Fixed  Sizfc_-_  263 

B.  Freely  Growing  Populations 281 

C.  Natural  Populations 300 

III.  Conclusion 325 

References 328 


General  Introduction 

Endocrine  adaptive  responses  have  become  of  particular  interest  to  the 
mammalogist  in  recent  years  because  of  the  likelihood  that  they  play  an 
important  role  in  the  regulation  of  the  growth  of  mammalian  populations. 
Sufficient  evidence  has  accumulated  from  the  field  and  laboratory  to  war- 
rant stating  with  fair  certainty  that  these  adaptive  mechanisms  are  opera- 
tive in  and  related  to  changes  in  the  size  of  mammalian  populations. 
However,  there  is  still  considerable  uncertainty  about  their  precise  role  and 
relative  importance  in  the  regulation  of  population  growth,  especially  with 
regard  to  generalizing  to  a  large  number  of  species  from  the  few  species  for 
which  data  are  presently  available.  There  also  exists  a  great  deal  of  un- 

*  Note  added  in  proof.  For  additional  references  pertaining  to  recent  work  on  endocrines 
and  population,  the  reader  is  referred  to  Christian  (1961,  1963a  &  b). 

189 


190  /•   /•   Christian 

certainty  about  the  relationships  of  various  environmental  factors  to  the 
adaptive  mechanisms.  Then,  besides  the  interest  in  adapti\-e  mechanisms  in 
relation  to  population  growth,  there  is  the  frequently  overlooked  fact  that 
these  same  phj^siologic  reactions  may  effect  profound  morphologic  changes 
in  the  members  of  a  population  and  therefore  directly  affect  the  taxonomist 
who  must  use  morphologic  criteria  for  distinguishing  species  and  subspecies. 
It  is  entirely  possible  that  many  subspecific  descriptions  have  been  based 
on  morphologic  differences  resultmg  from  differences  in  the  densities  of  the 
populations  on  which  the  descriptions  are  based.  The  mammalogist  inter- 
ested in  reproduction  in  mammals  must  take  adaptive  mechanisms  into 
consideration,  as  alterations  in  reproductive  functions  are  an  integral  part 
of  these  same  adaptive  responses.  Therefore  there  is  adequate  justification 
for  this  chapter  on  the  endocrine  adaptive  responses,  their  effects,  and  their 
relationships  to  the  densities  of  mammalian  populations. 

No  matter  how  well  an  animal  may  be  genetically  adapted  to  its  general 
environment,  it  still  must  have  sufficient  adaptive  flexibility  to  meet  the 
daily  and  seasonal  environmental  changes,  as  well  as  emergency  situations, 
to  which  it  will  normally  be  subjected,  and  still  maintain  a  constant  internal 
environment.  Nothing  in  the  daily  life  and  external  environment  of  an 
animal  remains  constant;  on  the  contrary  there  frequently  are  very  sudden, 
often  extreme,  shifts  in  the  environment  which  are  stimuli  that,  if  unop- 
posed, would  alter  the  internal  environment  of  the  animals.  But  the  internal 
environment  must  remain  constant  if  the  animal  is  to  survive.  Therefore 
there  must  be  a  constantly  active  system  of  physiologic  feedback  mecha- 
nisms to  compensate  immediately  for  any  tendencies  to  shift  the  internal 
environment.  However,  these  adaptive  responses  do  not  take  place  without 
producing  measurable  effects  in  the  organs  and  glands  primarily  responsible 
for  meeting  the  altered  demands.  Compensation  for  a  life-maintaining 
change  frequently  occurs  at  the  expense  of  some  function  less  immediately 
important  for  survival,  for  example,  reproduction.  Consequently  reproduc- 
tive function  declines  measurably  in  the  face  of  a  need  to  maintain  a  con- 
stant internal  physiologic  state  in  the  presence  of  adversity.  The  adaptive 
responses  are  changing  constantly  in  degree  to  meet  constantly  changing 
daily  circumslances,  and  it  is  generally  thought  that  a  certain  amount  of 
change  is  necessary  to  maintain  the  integrity  of  the  system  so  that  it  will 
be  capable  of  responding  to  more  demanding  circumstances.  It  is  not  sur- 
prising that  at  any  given  moment  the  physiologic  status  of  a  mammal 
reflects  its  total  environment  and  that  the  whole  system  is  in  a  constant 
stage  of  change,  but  for  these  same  reasons  it  becomes  difficult  to  study 
such  a  dynamic  system  in  the  complex  environments  of  natural  populations. 
Therefore  a  great  deal  of  the  existing  evidence  on  the  adaptive  mechanisms 
of  mammals,  especially  in  relation  to  population  density,  has  been  gained 
by  studies  in  the  laboratory. 


2.  Endocrines  and  Populations  191 

Past  research  on  the  adaptive  mechanisms  has  emphasized  the  adrenal 
cortices  and  to  a  lesser  extent  the  thyroid  gland  and  their  hormones.  Un- 
questionably the  adrenal  cortex  is  essential  to  life  and  plays  a  basic  role  in 
the  adaptive  responses,  nevertheless  there  is  a  real  tendency  to  overlook 
the  paramount  importance  of  other  systems  and  organs  which  also  respond 
to  adverse  circumstances.  In  fact  their  actions  are  simultaneous  with  and 
inseparable  from  the  actions  of  the  adrenal  cortex  in  many  instances.  A 
great  many  responses  on  the  part  of  the  organism  act  in  concert  to  prevent 
any  alteration  in  the  basic  physiology  of  the  animal  and  to  meet  emergency 
needs.  The  central  nervous  system  is  a  major  and  integral  part  of  these 
adaptive  mechanisms.  Our  understanding  and  interpretation  of  the  physio- 
logic changes  taking  place  under  a  given  set  of  circumstances  too  frequently 
suffer  from  a  tendency  to  think  statically  and  in  terms  of  isolated  organs, 
systems,  or  hormones — a  result  of  the  kind  of  experimental  approach 
necessary  to  understand  the  actions  of  various  glands  and  their  secretions. 
The  isolated  organ  concept  must  give  way  to  thinking  in  terms  of  dyna- 
mically interacting  systems.  However,  to  describe  these  mechanisms  and 
their  effects  in  dynamic  inclusive  terms  is  extremely  difficult  and  has  been 
made  even  more  so  by  the  recent  elucidation  of  the  key  role  played  by  the 
central  nervous  system  in  regulating  the  activities  of  the  glands  of  internal 
secretion,  as  well  as  by  the  realization  that  we  are  dealing  with  an  enor- 
mously complex  interacting  system  further  complicated  by  the  complex 
temporal  relationships  of  the  responses  of  these  systems  to  the  applied 
stimulus  and  to  each  other. 

The  balance  of  this  chapter  will  be  devoted  to  a  more  detailed  discussion, 
first  of  the  physiologic  adaptive  mechanisms  themselves,  and  then  with 
particular  attention  to  the  evidence  implicating  these  responses  in  the 
regulation  of  the  growth  of  mammalian  populations.  An  attempt  will  be 
made  to  clarify  some  of  these  responses  and  to  indicate  areas  where  further 
research  is  needed,  especially  in  relation  to  behavior  and  population  density. 
Subjects  which  are  adequately  covered  in  the  usual  textbooks  of  physiology 
will  be  omitted  or  only  briefly  summarized. 


Part  1.      The  Endocrine  Adaptive  Mechanisms 


I.  Introduction 

When  a  mammal  is  subjected  to  a  stimulus  which,  if  unopposed,  would 
result  at  least  in  a  change  in  its  internal  physiology,  and  more  likely  produce; 
a  circulatory  collapse,  a  series  of  neural,  neuroendocrine,  endocrme,  and 
vascular  responses  follow  which  counteract  the  deleterious  effects  of  the 


192  J-  J-   Christian 

stimulus  and  also  supply  the  increased  needs  of  many  tissues  in  order  to 
meet  the  situation.  Selye  (1950)  introduced  the  phrase  "alarm  stimulus"  to 
describe  such  a  stimulus  which  produces  shock  and  evokes  the  usual 
physiologic  responses  to  shock.  In  the  present  account  an  alarm  stimulus  is 
defined  as  any  stimulus  which,  when  applied  to  a  mammal,  tends  to  alter 
fluid  and  circulatory  homeostasis,  and  therefore  necessitates  a  physiologic 
adaptive  response.  This  definition  is  somewhat  circular  insofar  as  it  is  in 
terms  of  a  response,  but  it  is  not  restrictive,  and  it  does  not  imply  that  the 
adrenal  cortex  (at  least  that  part  responsible  for  the  secretion  of  carbo- 
hydrate-active corticoids)  is  an  essential  participant,  as  is  so  often  assumed. 
There  may  be  qualitative  similarities  in  the  responses  to  different  stimuli, 
but  detailed  studies  suggest  that  there  are  all  degrees  of  variation  in  the 
degree  of  participation  of  various  systems  and  organs  to  a  gi\'en  stimulus. 
Probably  the  prime  objection  to  the  current  concept  of  "nonspecific" 
response  is  the  practical  one  that  uncritical  usage  has  tended  to  obscure 
important  differences  in  the  physiologic  responses  to  different  stimuli. 

It  should  be  pointed  out  that  the  degree  of  these  responses  appears  to  be 
relative,  as  the  same  responses  qualitatively  are  essential  for  daily  life,  but 
must  increase  quantitatively  in  the  face  of  adverse  circumstances.  A  pri- 
mary function  of  the  endocrine  adaptive  responses  is  to  insure  an  adequate 
circulation  with  an  adequate  supply  of  glucose  and  oxygen  to  tissues  essen- 
tial for  emergency  situations.  Part  of  this  function  is  the  maintenance  of  an 
adequate  circulatory  volume  and  proper  electrolyte  and  fluid  balances. 
These  adaptive  responses  will  be  discussed  in  greater  detail  in  the  following 
account. 


II.  The  Endocrine  Glands  of  Adaptation 
A.  The  Adrenal  Glands 

1.  Introduction 

This  discussion  is  primarily  for  the  benefit  of  those  who  are  interested  in 
the  physiological  and  comparative  aspects  of  mammalogy.  Therefore  what 
is  known  of  the  endocrine  adaptive  mechanisms  will  be  outlined  without 
dwelling  on  details  or  becoming  involved  in  the  minor  details  or  contro- 
versies of  today's  frontiers  in  endocrinology. 

Research  on  adaptive  mechanisms  to  a  large  extent  has  centered  around 
the  adrenal  glands,  especially  the  cortex.  One  of  the  factors  tending  to 
synonomize  "stress"  with  adrenocortical  activity  has  been  the  measure- 
ment steroid  secretion,  weight,  ascorbic  acid  depletion,  and  cholesterol 


2.  Endocrines  and  Populations  193 

content  of  the  adrenals  to  determine  whether  and  to  what  degree  a  stunulus 
produces  ''stress."  Although  the  adrenals  are  of  primary  importance  in 
physiologic  adaptation  to  changing  needs,  it  is  important  not  to  equate 
adaptation  solely  with  adrenal  function  or  to  assume  that  the  only  function 
of  the  adrenals  is  to  enable  an  organism  to  meet  new  and  sudden  demands. 
It  is  appropriate,  especially  for  the  mammalogist,  to  discuss  adrenal  glands 
in  some  detaU  because  of  their  importance  and  because  of  the  convenience 
of  using  them  as  indices  of  the  degree  of  adaptive  response  to  particular 
situations  or  stimuli.  However,  judgment  must  be  used  in  interpreting  the 
results  of  measurements  of  adrenal  function,  and  one  must  realize  that 
there  are  many  other  responses  which  are  measured  with  extreme  difficulty, 
and  yet  others  may  be  completely  masked  by  extraneous  factors. 

a.  General  morphology  of  the  adrenal  glands.  The  anatomy  of  the  adrenal 
glands  is  discussed  in  detail  in  many  texts  and  papers  on  histology,  gross 
anatomy,  and  comparative  anatomy.  Attention  is  called  to  the  books  by 
Bourne  (1949),  Hartman  and  Brownell  (1949),  Bachman  (1954),  and 
Jones  (1957)  for  general  treatments,  especially  from  the  comparative  point 
of  view. 

The  adrenal  glands  are  yellowish  paired  organs  lying  at  or  near  the 
anterior  poles  of  the  kidneys.  Their  position  and  form  vary  considerably 
from  species  to  species.  For  example,  in  rabbits  (Stjlvilagus,  Oryctolagus) 
they  are  oval  discoid  organs  closely  applied  to  the  vena  cava;  in  wood- 
chucks  (Marmota)  they  are  sausage-shaped  and  lie  between  the  kidneys 
and  the  midline,  usually  closer  to  the  latter;  in  mice  and  voles  of  almost  all 
species  they  are  round,  oval,  or  pyramidal  and  lie  approximated  to  the 
poles  of  the  kidneys;  and  in  the  bats  Myotis  and  Pipistrellus  they  lie  be- 
neath a  layer  of  the  renal  capsule.  These  examples  simply  serve  to  illustrate 
the  wide  variations  that  occur  in  their  gross  shape  and  position. 

Two  distinct  portions  of  the  adrenal  are  discernible  when  they  are  sec- 
tioned and  examined  grossly:  a  dark  reddish  brown  or  gray  central  core, 
the  medulla ;  and  a  wide  outer  portion,  the  cortex,  which  is  usually  yellowish 
but  may  be  gray  or  even  translucent  reddish  brown,  dependmg  on  the 
activity  of  the  gland.  The  yellow  color  is  imparted  by  lipids  contained  in 
the  cortical  cells;  thus  color  will  vary  with  changes  in  the  lipid  content. 
Usually  the  cortex  is  quite  wide,  comprising  from  one-half  to  two-thirds  of 
the  radius  of  the  gland.  However,  in  some  of  the  adult  soricid  shrews  (Sorex 
jumeus,  S.  cinereus,  S.  palustris,  S.  dispar,  and  Microsorex  hoyi)  the  gland 
consists  almost  entirely  of  medulla  and  has  a  very  narrow  cortex  only  a  few 
cells  wide.  The  extreme  narrowness  of  the  cortex  is  especially  pronounced 
in  mature  male  shrews. 

The  adrenal  gland  is  surrounded  by  a  connective  tissue  capsule  from 
which  a  stromal  framework  of  connective  tissue  descends  into  the  cortex. 


194  /•  /•   Christian 

The  amount  of  cortical  stroma  may  vary  considerably;  it  is  inconspicuous 
in  most  rodents  whereas  it  is  marked  in  most  carnivores. 

b.  Zonation  of  the  adrenal  cortex.  Three  distinct  major  zones  usually  are 
identifiable  histologically  in  the  cortex,  although  the  zonation  is  difficult  to 
discern  in  a  number  of  species  (Bourne,  1949) .  An  outer  thin  zona  glomeru- 
losa  lies  just  beneath  the  capsule  and  consists  of  loops  or  balls  of  rather 
large  cells  with  relatively  clear  cytoplasm.  Central  to  the  zona  glomerulosa 
is  a  wide  central  zona  fasciculata,  which  is  composed  of  radially  arranged 
straight  cords  of  polyhedral  cells  that  usually  contain  numerous  cytoplasmic 
lipid  vacuoles.  Lipid  vacuoles  occur  in  the  cortical  cells  of  most  species, 
but  they  may  be  absent  in  some,  for  example,  the  golden  hamster  (il/c.so- 
cricetus  auratus)  (Alpert,  1950;  Knigge,  1954a;  Schindler  and  Knigge, 
1959a).  Little  or  no  lipid  is  present  in  the  cortices  of  cattle,  sheep,  and 
pigs  (Deane  and  Seligman,  1953).  When  present,  the  vacuoles  may  vary 
considerably  in  size  and  number,  depending  on  variations  in  the  activity 
of  the  cortex.  The  cells  of  the  outer  half  of  the  zona  fasciculata  usually  are 
larger  and  contain  more  lipid  than  those  in  the  inner  half  of  the  zone.  The 
fascicular  cords  are  arranged  as  paired  columns  of  cells  lining  vascular 
sinusoids  in  man  and  monkeys  (Elias  and  Pauly,  1956),  but  is  continuous 
in  rats,  the  sinusoids  penetrating  the  continuum  (Pauly,  1957).  The  latter 
normally  contain  large  amounts  of  blood  circulating  from  the  arteries  in 
the  capsule  to  the  medullary  venous  sinusoids  and  adrenal  vein.  There  are 
variations  in  the  circulatory  arrangement  with  species,  and  it  is  more  com- 
plex in  detail  than  has  been  described  here,  but  these  matters  are  thoroughly 
covered  elsewhere  (Gersh  and  Grollman,  1941;  Hartman  andBrownell, 
1949;  Harrison,  1951,  1957;  Elias  and  Pauly,  1956;  Pauly,  1957).  The  cells 
of  the  fasciculata,  when  stamed  by  routine  procedures,  bear  a  marked 
resemblance  to  the  luteal  cells  of  the  ovary,  interstitial  cells  of  the  testis, 
and,  although  less  closely,  the  cells  of  the  "brown  fat"  or  "hibernating 
gland"  in  its  usual  functional  state.  The  zona  reticularis  forms  a  fairly  wide 
cortical  band  between  the  medulla  and  the  zona  fasciculata  in  most  species, 
but  it  is  not  always  present  (Hartman  and  Brownell,  1949).  Its  cords  (or 
cortical  continuum)  are  more  or  less  continuous  peripherally  with  those  of 
the  zona  fasciculata,  but  they  rapidly  break  up  into  a  reticular  network  as 
they  proceed  centrally  toward  the  medulla.  The  cells  are  generally  smaller 
than  other  cortical  cells  and  usually  contain  no  vacuoles.  However,  when 
vacuoles  are  present,  they  are  usually  very  large. 

There  is  need  for  a  detailed,  well  illustrated,  and  thorough  discussion  of 
the  comparative  morphology  of  the  adrenal  glands  which  would  include  a 
wide  variety  of  species  and  a  sufficient  number  of  animals  of  each  species 
to  describe  age  and  sex,  as  well  as  seasonal  and  environmental,  relation- 
ships. It  is  not  the  purpose  of  the  present  discussion  to  dwell  on  the  anatomy 


2.  Endocrines  and  Populations  195 

of  the  adrenal  glands,  but  a  brief  summary  of  the  particularly  useful  and 
more  recent  publications  on  the  subject  will  be  given.  The  books  which 
already  have  been  listed  discuss  the  adrenals  of  a  large  number  of  species, 
but  the  descriptions  and  illustrations  are  limited.  However,  the  histology 
and  histochemistry  of  the  adrenals  of  a  few  species  are  discussed  in  con- 
siderable detail  in  a  number  of  papers.  The  recent  publications  of  Elias  and 
Pauly  (1956)  and  Pauly  (1957)  describe  the  microscopic  anatomy  of  the 
adrenal  glands  of  laboratory  rats  and  humans.  These  papers  are  well 
illustrated,  and  the  stereographic  reconstructions  of  serial  sections  are 
helpful  in  understanding  the  adrenal  morphology  of  these  two  species.  One 
of  the  important  facts  brought  out  in  these  papers  is  that  the  adrenal  cortex 
of  the  rat  is  not  arranged  in  cords  as  it  is  in  humans  and  monkeys.  The 
parenchyma  of  the  rat  adrenal  cortex  is  a  continuum  which  is  tunneled  by 
vascular  channels.  A  number  of  additional  papers  deal  with  the  anatomy, 
circulation,  or  histochemistry  of  the  adrenals  of  laboratory  rats,  especially 
with  regard  to  function,  zonation,  and  reactions  to  various  stimuli  (Howard, 
1938;  Flexner  and  Grollman,  1939;  Greep  and  Doane,  1947,  1949a;  Deane 
et  al.,  1948;  Deane  and  Morse,  1948;  Cain  and  Harrison,  1950;  Feldman, 
1950,  1951;  Cater  and  Stack-Dvmne,  1953,  1955;  Josimovich  et  al,  1954; 
Jones  and  Spalding,  1954;  Jones  and  Wright,  1954a,  b;  Christianson  and 
Jones,  1957),  and  other  more  general  papers  on  the  histochemistry  and 
function  of  the  adrenals  are  based  largely  on  material  from  laboratory  rats 
(Dempsey,  1948;  Greep  and  Deane,  1949a,  b;  Sayers  and  Sayers,  1949). 
The  differences  in  morphology  between  the  adrenals  of  wild  rats  {Rattus 
norvegicus  and  Rattus  alexandrinus)  and  those  of  Norway  rats  from  the 
laboratory  have  been  described  by  Rogers  and  Richter  (1948),  and  the 
histology  of  wild  and  laboratory  Norway  rats  has  been  described  and  com- 
pared by  Mosier  (1957).  A  comparative  study  of  the  vascularization  of 
the  adrenals  of  rabbits,  rats,  and  cats  has  been  made  by  Harrison  (1951) 
and  followed  by  a  description  of  the  adrenal  circulation  and  its  regulation 
in  the  laboratory  rabbit  (Onjctolagus)  (Harrison,  1957). 

The  histology  of  the  adrenal  glands  of  the  prototherians  Onuthorlnjnchus 
and  Tachyglossus  has  been  described  in  considerable  detail  by  Wright  et  al. 
(1957).  The  bulk  of  the  chromaffin  tissue  was  found  in  the  lower  pole  in 
these  species  rather  than  in  the  more  usual  central  position.  The  cortices  of 
these  species  also  differ  considerably  in  their  histologic  appearance  from 
those  of  eutherians.  We  have  mentioned  above  that  the  adrenals  of  North 
American  soricids  have  strikingly  little  cortical  tissue,  although  a  critical 
study  of  this  material  has  not  been  made  (J.  J.  Christian,  unpublished) . 
Lanman  (1957)  has  described  the  fetal  zones  of  the  adrenals  of  the  fol- 
lowing fetal  or  neonatal  primates:  macques  (Macaca  midatta) ,  potto 
{Perodicus  putto) ,  chimpanzee  (Pan  sp.),  hybrids  of  Cercopithecus  {Cerco- 


196  /•  J-  Christian 

pithecus  sp.),  marmoset  {Callothrix  argentata) ,  slow  loris  (Loris  sp.),  colo- 
bus  monkey  {Colohus  polykomos),  and  humans.  The  anatomy  of  the 
adrenals  of  the  macaque  has  been  described  by  Harrison  and  Asling  (1955) . 
Variations  in  the  histochemistry  of  the  adrenals  of  cows,  rats,  and  monkeys 
followmg  "stress"  or  treatment  with  adrenocorticotropin,  cortisone,  or 
deoxycorticosterone  were  the  subject  of  a  paper  by  Glick  and  Ochs  (1955) . 
Additional  descriptions  of  the  adrenals  of  cows  and  other  domesticated 
ungulates  are  subjects  of  papers  by  Elias  (1948)  and  Weber  et  al.,  (1950). 
Finally,  Zalesky  (1934)  described  in  considerable  detail  the  seasonal 
histologic  changes  in  the  adrenals  of  thirteen-lined  ground  squirrels  ( Citel- 
lus  tridecemlineatus) . 

The  morphology  and  histochemistry  of  the  adrenals  of  laboratory  and 
wild  house  mice  have  been  thoroughly  studied,  largely  because  of  the 
endocrine  relationships  of  the  transitorj^  X-zone  which  was  first  described 
by  Howard  (1927).  Tamura  (1926)  wrote  a  detailed  description  of  the 
changes  during  pregnancy  in  the  adrenals  of  mice.  This  was  followed  by 
Howard's  (1927)  description  of  the  X-zone  and  Waring's  (1935)  descrip- 
tion of  the  development  of  the  adrenal  glands  of  the  mouse.  Following 
these  there  was  a  spate  of  papers  describing  the  X-zone  and  its  reactions 
to  various  hormones  and  experimental  treatments  (Gersh  andGrollman, 
1939;  Waring,  1942;  McPhail  and  Read,  1942a,  b;  McPhail,  1944;  Jones, 
1948,  1949a,  b,  1950,  1952;  Miller,  1949;  Benua  and  Howard,  1950;  Howard 
and  Benua,  1950;  Jones  and  Roby,  1954;  Allen,  1954;  Allen,  1957).  The 
histology  and  histophysiology  of  the  adrenals  of  hamsters  (Mesocricetus 
auratus)  have  been  described  by  Alpert  (1950)  and  Holmes  (1955),  and 
the  effects  of  hypophysectomy  and  starvation  on  their  adrenals  by  Knigge 
(1954a,  b). 

The  adrenals  of  a  number  of  species  of  European  small  mammals  have 
been  studied  and  described  by  Delost,  particular  attention  being  paid  to 
the  presence  or  absence  of  an  X-zone  and  its  relationships  to  the  sexual 
cycle  and  sex  accessories.  The  mammals  in  these  studies  included  Microtus 
arvalis  (Delost  1951;  1952a,  b;  1954;  1956a,  b)  Microtus  agrestis  (Delost 
and  Delost,  1955),  Clethrionomijs  glareolus  (Delost  and  Delost,  1954), 
Pitymys  (Delost  and  Delost,  1955),  Sorex  araneus  (Delost,  1957),  and 
Crocidura  (Delost,  1957). 

Immature  male  and  young  nuUiparous  female  house  mice  (Mus  muscu- 
lus)  have  an  adrenocortical  juxtamedullary  zone,  the  X-zone,  which  un- 
equivocally shows  sex  relationships  (Howard,  1927;  Deanesly,  1928;  Jones, 
1957) .  This  zone  is  absent  from  mature  male  and  parous  or  old  females. 
Cortical  X-zones  have  been  described  for  a  number  of  other  species  of 
small  mammals  including  meadow  voles  (Microtus  agrestis  and  Microtus 
arvalis),  red-backed  voles   (Clcthrionomys  glareolus,  pine  voles   (Pitymys 


2.  Endocrines  and  Populations  197 

suhterraneiis) ,  and  shrews  (Sorex  araneus  and  Crocidura  russula)  (Delost, 
1951,  1952a,  1954,  1957;  Delost  and  Delost,  1954,  1955),  but  there  is  some 
question  whether  the  X-zones  of  these  species  are  entirely  analogous  to  the 
X-zones  of  the  house  mouse.  Delost  (1954,  1956b)  reports  that  cortisone 
involutes  the  so-called  X-zone  of  voles,  which  is  a  response  not  seen  in 
house  mice.  The  X-zone  of  the  shrew  behaves  like  that  of  the  house  mouse 
with  respect  to  its  involution,  but  apparently  has  not  been  subjected  to 
critical  experiments  in  the  laboratory  (Delost,  1957).  The  X-zone  consists 
of  cords  of  small,  deeply  acidophilic  cells  with  intensely  basophilic  nuclei 
and  which  are  about  one-half  the  size  of  those  of  the  zona  fasciculata 
(Howard,  1927;  Deanesly,  1928;  Jones,  1949a,  b,  1950,  1957;  Benua  and 
Howard,  1950) .  The  cytoplasm  of  these  cells,  besides  being  more  acido- 
philic than  those  of  the  fascicular  cells,  is  unvacuolated  ordinarily  and 
lacks  the  sudanophilia  of  the  other  zones  of  the  adrenal  cortex  (Jones, 
1957) .  Criteria  for  critically  distinguishing  the  X-zone  have  been  reviewed 
by  Benua  and  Howard  (1950)  and  Holmes  (1955).  The  uniqueness  of  this 
zone  rests  on  the  fact  that  it  is  involuted  by  androgens  and  appears  to  de- 
pend on  pituitary  luteinizing  hormone  for  its  maintenance  (Howard,  1927, 
1959;  McPhail  and  Read,  1942a,  b;  McPhail,  1944;  Waring,  1942;  Jones, 
1949a,  b,  1950,  1952,  1957).  The  function  of  this  zone,  if  there  is  a  specific 
function,  is  unknown.  The  so-called  X-zone  of  voles  which  has  been  de- 
scribed by  Delost  (1951,  1952a,  1954;)  and  Delost  and  Delost  (1954,  1955) 
reappears  after  castration  or  after  the  hibernal  periods  of  sexual  inactivity 
in  the  males  and  persists  through  gestation  and  lactation  in  the  females, 
and  in  these  respects  it  differs  markedly  from  the  X-zone  of  house  mice. 
This  zone  may  confound  the  use  of  adrenal  weight  as  an  index  of  increased 
cortical  activity  in  the  house  mouse  (Christian,  1956)  and  other  species 
which  possess  it,  but  it  provides  a  useful  measurement  for  determining 
histologically  the  onset  of  androgen  production,  therefore  puberty,  in  male 
house  mice  (Christian,  1956).  A  poorly  defined  X-zone  has  been  described 
in  mature  nulliparous  female  hamsters,  but  not  in  males  (Holmes,  1955), 
differing  from  the  X-zone  of  house  mice  in  this  respect.  It  is  likely  that  an 
X-zone  will  be  described  for  other  species  when  enough  material  from  all 
age  groups  of  both  sexes  has  been  critically  examined,  and  that  a  variety  of 
manifestations  of  this  zone  will  be  found. 

The  morphology  and  size  of  the  adrenal  cortex  varies  with  its  functional 
status  (see  also  discussion  under  reticularis) .  The  cortex  undergoes  rapid 
hyperplasia  and  hypertrophy  in  response  to  stimulation  by  adrenocorti- 
cotropin  (ACTH)  from  the  anterior  pituitary.  At  first  there  is  a  rapid 
diminution  in  the  size  and  number  of  lipid  vacuoles,  ascorbic  acid,  and 
cholesterol  of  the  cortical  cells  (Sayers  and  Sayers,  1949).  The  vacuoles 
soon  increase  in  number  and  size,  providng  the  stimulation  is  not  too 


198  ./.  J.   Christian 

severe  (Dempsey,  1948;  Sayers  and  Sayers,  1949;  Greep  and  Deane, 
1949b) .  The  lipid  vacuoles  disappear  first  from  the  fasciculata  next  to  the 
reticularis,  so  that  it  becomes  indistinguishable  from  the  latter.  As  stimula- 
tion continues,  the  disappearance  continues  centrifugally,  and  at  the  same 
time  enzymes  normally  absent  from  the  fasciculata,  but  present  in  the 
reticularis,  make  their  appearance  in  the  cells  of  the  fasciculata,  the  inner- 
most portion  moving  outward  (Symington  et  at.,  1958).  Upon  withdrawal 
of  the  stimulus  of  ACTH  the  lipid  vacuoles  increase  considerably  in  size 
and  may  become  very  large.  This  stage  presumably  represents  lipid  storage. 
The  cellular  hyperplasia  and  hypertrophy  mainly  account  for  increases  in 
the  size  and  weight  of  the  adrenal  glands.  Initially  the  cortex  responds  to 
stimulation  with  a  marked  decline  in  its  cholesterol,  neutral  lipids,  and 
ascorbic  acid  content  (Greep  and  Deane,  1949b;  Sayers  and  Sayers,  1949). 
These  soon  return  at  least  partially  to  their  original  state,  and  in  the  inac- 
tive gland  they  may  exceed  their  original  levels.  These  matters  are  dis- 
cussed in  detail  in  the  cited  references  in  addition  to  discussions  therein  of 
the  relationships  of  the  cortex  and  its  activity  to  various  stimuli  for  varying 
lengths  of  time  and  with  varying  intensity. 

c.  The  adrenal  medulla.  The  adrenal  medulla  consists  of  rather  irregular 
masses  of  polyhedral  chromaffin  cells  derived,  along  with  the  ganglia  of 
the  sympathetic  nervous  system,  from  the  primitive  neuroectoderm.  The 
medulla  is  homologous  with  the  sympathetic  ganglia  and  receives  myeli- 
nated cholinergic  preganglionic  fibers  from  the  greater  splanchnic  nerve. 
The  medulla  itself  serves  as  the  ganglion  and  the  postganglionic  tracts. 
There  apparently  are  several  types  of  cells  in  the  medulla;  these  are  dis- 
cussed in  more  detail  elsewhere  (Hartman  and  Brownell,  1949;  Eranko  and 
Raisanen,  1957) .  The  cytoplasm  of  the  medullary  cells  contains  numerous 
minute  deeply  basophilic  granules  which  stain  blue  with  ferric  chloride 
and  brown  with  chromic  acid  (chromaffin)  and  which  appear  in  some  way 
to  be  related  to  secretory  function. 

The  adrenal  medulla  generally  is  not  thought  to  hypertrophy  following 
stimulation  in  the  same  way  that  the  adrenal  cortex  does.  Rogers  and 
Richter  (1948)  reported  the  absence  of  medullary  hypertrophy  with 
changes  in  adrenal  size  in  rats.  However,  there  is  good  evidence  that  the 
medulla  does  hypertrophy,  at  least  in  some  species  and  under  some  circum- 
stances, even  though  it  may  not  contribute  significantly  to  an  increase  in 
the  total  weight  of  the  gland,  as  a  consideration  of  its  geometry  will  show. 
House  mice  have  been  shown  to  exhibit  a  marked  medullary  hyperplasia 
and  hypertrophy  during  pregnancy  (Tamura,  1926)  or  chronic  stimulation 
due  to  crowding  (Bullough,  1952).  Medullary  hypertrophy  also  has  been 
observed  in  a  variety  of  species  of  captive  wild  ungulates  subjected  to 
conditions  in  a  zoological  garden  similar  to  the  crowding  of  mice  reported 


2.  Endocrines  and  Populations  199 

by  Biillough  ( 19o2)  (Ratcliffe,  unpublished;  J.  J.  Christian,  unpubUshed) . 
These  conditions  which  resulted  in  which  medullary  hypertrophy  all 
constituted  prolonged,  chronic  stimuli.  Medullary  hypertrophy  due  to 
hyperplasia  probably  occurs  simultaneously  with  cortical  hypertrophy  in 
many  species  but  perhaps  requires  a  more  sustained  stimulus  and  develops 
at  a  much  slower  rate.  There  also  seems  to  be  some  suggestion  that  emo- 
tional stimuli  may  be  important  in  this  effect.  Finally,  it  has  been  shown 
that  treatment  with  pituitary  growth  hormone  will  produce  a  marked 
hypertrophy  of  the  adrenal  medulla  (Moon  et  at.,  1951;  Lostroh  and  Li, 
1958) ,  and  may  eventually  result  in  medullary  tumors  (Moon  et  at.,  1950) . 
The  role  of  sympathicomedullary  function  in  physiologic  adaptation  re- 
quires more  investigation,  especially  in  regard  to  chronic  stimulation,  such 
as  is  produced  by  sociopsychologic  pressures,  and  for  a  variety  of  species. 

2.  Hormones  Secreted  by  the  Adrenal  Cortex:  Their  Actions  and 
THE  Regulation  of  Their  Secretion. 

a.  The  zona  glomerulosa.  (1)  The  hormones.  The  adrenal  cortex  secretes 
two  steroid  hormones,  aldosterone  (18-aldocorticosterone)  and  deoxycorti- 
costerone (11-deoxycorticosterone),  which  have  their  primary  effects  on 
salt-electrolyte  and  water  metabolism.  However,  aldosterone  is  the  only  bio- 
logically important  sodium-retaining  corticoid  secreted  by  the  adrenal  cor- 
tex and  it  is  many  times  more  powerful  than  deoxycorticosterone  in  its 
effects  on  electrolyte  metabolism  (Farrell  et  al.,  1955;  Gaunt  et  at.,  1955; 
Gross  and  Lichtlen,  1958) .  Also  aldosterone  is  an  important  secretory  pro- 
duct of  the  adrenal  cortex,  whereas  deoxycorticosterone  is  secreted  only  in 
trace  amounts  (Farrell  et  at.,  1955;  Jones,  1957)  and  is  probably  a  precursor 
in  the  formation  of  aldosterone  (Giroud  et  al.,  1958).  The  actions  of  these 
two  hormones  are  very  similar  within  the  physiological  range  of  dosages  for 
each,  but  their  actions  with  overdosage  differ  considerably:  overdosage 
with  aldosterone  does  not  lead  to  the  excessive  sodium  retention  and  the 
diabetes  insipidus-like  state  which  are  seen  after  overdosage  with  deoxy- 
corticosterone (Gross  and  Lichtlen,  1958). 

It  is  appropriate  at  this  point  to  comment  on  the  general  classification  of 
the  adrenal  corticoids  into  the  two  broad  categories  which  are  used  in  the 
present  account.  The  hormones  of  the  adrenal  cortex  have  been  loosely 
grouped  as  "sodium-retaining"  or  ''carbohydrate-active"  according  to 
whether  their  prunary  actions  are  on  salt-electrolyte  metabolism  or  if  they 
are  among  those  steroids  having  marked  effects  on  carbohydrate  meta- 
bolism. The  sodium-retaining  steroids  include  aldosterone,  deoxycorti- 
costerone, and,  to  a  much  lesser  extent,  17-hydroxy-ll-deoxycorticosterone 
(Reichstein's  compound  S) .  The  principal  carbohydrate-active  steroids  are 


200  J.  J.   Christian 

hydrocortisone  and  cortisone  (ll-oxy-17-hydroxycorticoids).  Corticoste- 
rone  is  included  in  this  latter  group,  although  it  has  moderate  effects  on 
both  salt-electrolyte  and  carbohydrate  metabolism.  It  is  considerably 
weaker  in  all  these  actions  than  the  principal  corticoids  in  either  of  the 
categories  (Jones,  1957).  This  classification  into  primarily  carbohydrate- 
active  and  primarily  sodium-retaining  corticoids  is  useful,  but  by  no  means 
does  it  reflect  the  entire  spectrum  of  activities  of  the  hormones;  in  many 
instances  there  is  a  considerable  overlap  in  these  activities  for  a  particular 
hormone,  for  example,  corticosterone. 

Recent  morphologic  and  direct  evidence  shows  that  the  secretion  of 
aldosterone  is  a  function  of  the  zona  glomerulosa,  whereas  the  carbohy- 
drate-active corticoids,  except  corticosterone,  and  probably  the  C19  steroids 
are  secreted  by  the  zona  fasciculata  and  zona  reticularis.  Probably  the 
most  conclusive  evidence  for  the  relationship  between  specific  secretory 
function  and  zonation  of  the  adrenal  cortex  has  been  provided  by  the  in 
vitro  incubation  and  determination  of  the  secretory  products  of  selected 
segments  of  the  adrenal  cortex.  Aldosterone  was  found  to  be  secreted  only 
by  incubated  portions  of  the  zona  glomerulosa  of  the  adrenals  of  rats  and 
beef  cattle  (Ayres  et  al.,  1956;  Giroud  et  al.,  1956;  Giroud  et  at.,  1958); 
hydrocortisone  was  produced  only  by  the  zonae  fasciculata-reticularis,  and 
corticosterone  was  produced  at  approximately  equal  rates  by  all  three 
zones  of  the  adrenals  of  beef  cattle  (Ayres  et  al.,  1956;  Giroud  ct  al.,  1958 
Stachenko  and  Giroud,  1959a,  b) .  It  was  also  shown  in  these  experiments 
that  ACTH  or  corticotropin  peptides  or  other  steroids  were  without  effect 
on  the  production  of  aldosterone  by  the  zona  glomerulosa  but  that  they 
markedly  increased  the  production  of  total  corticosteroids  and  of  hydro- 
cortisone by  the  fasciculata-reticularis  (Stachenko  and  Giroud,  1959b). 
Additional  evidence  of  functional  zonation,  less  direct,  has  been  obtained 
by  relating  changes  in  the  composition  of  the  secretory  product  with  mor- 
phologic changes  in  the  various  zones  of  the  adrenal  cortex.  A  sodimn- 
deficient  diet  produces  extreme  hypertrophy  of  the  zona  glomerulosa  and 
atrophy  of  the  zona  fasciculata  of  the  adrenal  cortices  of  rats  (Hartroft  and 
Eisenstein,  1957),  and  these  changes  are  associated  with  a  marked  increase 
in  the  secretion  of  aldosterone  and  decreases  in  the  secretion  of  corticoste- 
rone (Eisenstein  and  Hartroft,  1957).  In  somewhat  comparable  experi- 
ments it  was  shown  that  (1)  sodium  deprivation  markedly  increased  the 
aliesterase  activity  of  the  zona  glomerulosa  but  had  no  effect  on  its  activity 
in  the  fasciculata  of  the  adrenals  of  mice;  (2)  deoxycorticosterone  or  sodium 
flooding  depressed  the  aliesterase  activity  of  the  zona  glomerulosa  and 
increased  it  in  the  zona  fasciculata;  and  (3)  injected  ACTH  markedly 
increased  the  aliesterase  activity  of  the  zona  fasciculata  but  did  not  affect 
it  in  the  zona  glomerulosa,  whereas  blocking  ACTH  secretion  with  cortisone 


2.  Endocrines  and  Populations  201 

depressed  the  fascicular  aliesterase  activity  (Allen,  1957).  The  results  of 
the  foregoing  experiments  provide  convincing  evidence  in  support  of  the 
hypothesis  of  Greep  and  his  co-workers  that  the  zona  glomerulosa  secretes 
the  electrolyte-active,  and  the  fasciculata  the  carbohydrate-active,  corti- 
coids  (Greep  and  Deane,  1947,  1949b;  Deane  et  at.,  1948).  This  hypothesis 
was  based  on  observations  that  (1)  increased  sodium  intake  or  injections 
of  deoxycorticosterone  produced  histochemical  changes  indicative  of  de- 
creased activity  in  the  zona  glomerulosa  of  the  adrenals  of  rats,  and  that 
(2)  a  reduction  in  sodium  or  increase  in  potassium  produced  cytological 
changes  indicative  of  increased  activity  of  the  zona  glomerulosa.  There  is 
little  doubt  that  the  glomerulosa  is  responsible  primarily  for  the  secretion  of 
aldosterone  and  that  the  carbohydrate-active  corticoids  are  secreted  by 
the  zona  fasciculata  and  possibly  by  the  zona  reticularis.  Convincing  evi- 
dence of  a  functional  separation  between  the  zonae  fasciculata  and  reticu- 
laris is  not  available,  but  the  reticularis  generally  is  not  believed  to  be  as 
active  a  secretory  zone  as  the  fasciculata. 

The  chief  action  of  aldosterone  is  on  sodium-potassium  transport  in  the 
tubular  cells  of  the  renal  nephron,  and  it  is  relatively  more  effective  in  pro- 
moting sodium  retention  than  in  promoting  potassium  excretion  or  water 
retention  (Gaunt  et  al,  1955;  Bartter,  1956;  Jones,  1957;  Gross  and 
Lichtlen,  1958;  Stanbury  et  al.,  1958).  It  apparently  stimulates  the  ionic 
exchange  between  potassium  and  sodium  ions  in  the  renal  tubular  cells 
(Bartter,  1956;  Stanbury  et  al.,  1958),  although  an  overdosage  of  aldoste- 
rone will  not  produce  excessive  sodium  retention  and  the  animal  therefore 
stays  in  sodium  balance  (Bartter,  1956;  Gross  and  Lichtlen,  1958).  In 
general  the  sodium-retaining  corticoids  act  on  the  nephric  tubular  cells  to 
promote  an  ionic  exchange  between  sodium  and  potassium;  so  that  sodium 
is  retained  and  potassium  is  excreted  (Bartter,  1956,  1957).  Water  is  re- 
absorbed with  the  sodium  or  independently  under  the  action  of  neurohy- 
pophyseal antidiuretic  hormone  (ADH)  (Bartter,  1957).  Proper  fluid  and 
electrolyte  balance  is  mamtauied  by  these  homeostatic  endocrine  activities 
acting  in  concert  with  water  and  salt  intake  and  with  hemodynamic  and 
neural  factors  which  affect  fluid  volume,  blood  pressure,  and  renal  glomeru- 
lar filtration.  The  apparent  anomaly  of  overdosages  of  aldosterone  failing 
to  produce  excessive  sodium  retention  depends  on  the  fact  that  the  blood 
pressure  is  raised  and  therefore  the  glomerular  filtration  rate  is  increased 
and  sodium  is  lost  accordingly  (Stanbury  et  al,  1958) .  Proper  fluid  and 
electrolyte  balance  is  vital  to  any  animal,  and  adrenalectomized  animals 
can  be  maintained  with  injected  deoxycorticosterone  or  aldosterone,  al- 
though they  cannot  adapt  to  added  stress  (Gaunt  et  al,  1955) .  The  adrenal- 
ectomized laboratory  rat  or  mouse  also  can  be  maintained  alive  by  sup- 
plying W(  sodium  chloride  in  its  drinking  water  to  replace  the  sodium  loss 


202  J.  J.   Christian 

accompanying  adrenalectomy.  However,  adrenalectomized  wild  Norway 
rats  (Rattus  norvegicus)  cannot  be  maintained  in  this  fashion,  even  with 
the  NaCl  content  of  the  drinking  water  as  high  as  4  %(Richter  et  al.,  1950) . 
These  facts  emphasize  the  wide  divergence  between  laboratory  and  wild 
strains  of  the  same  species.  Evidently  the  requirements  for  adrenocortical 
hormones  are  much  greater  in  mammals  under  feral  conditions  than  for 
those  raised  or  maintained  in  the  laboratory  or  zoo.  There  is  a  marked 
disparity  in  the  adrenal  weights  of  mammals  raised  in  the  laboratory  and 
in  the  same  species  under  natural  conditions,  the  differences  due  largely  to 
differences  in  the  amount  of  cortical  tissue  (Rogers  and  Richter,  1948; 
Nichols,  1950;  Christian  and  Ratcliffe,  1952;  Christian,  1955a) ;  some  of  this 
difference,  however,  may  be  associated  with  the  unconscious  selection  in 
breeding  colonies  for  docility  and  good  breeding  performance. 

(2)  Regulation  of  aldosterone  secretion}  Since  aldosterone  acts  pri- 
marily to  maintain  fluid  and  electrolyte  homeostasis,  it  is  not  surprising 
that  the  secretion  of  this  hormone  is  regulated  largely  by  these  factors. 
Changes  in  the  volume  of  extracellular  fluid  (probably  mainly  the  intra- 
vascular volume) ,  and  the  level  of  body  potassium  affect  the  rate  of  aldo- 
sterone secretion  (Liddle  et  al.,  1956;  Bartter,  1957;  Bartter  et  at.,  1959), 
but  to  some  extent  the  secretion  of  aldosterone  in  vivo  can  be  stimulated 
by  adrenocorticotropin  (Farrell  et  al.,  1955;  1958;  Liddle  et  al.,  1956),  but 
apparently  not  in  vitro  (Stachenko  and  Giroud,  19596).  This  discrepancy 
may  be  explained  by  the  increased  production  of  the  precursors  of  aldoste- 
rone by  the  f  asciculata  which  then  become  accessible  to  the  zona  glomerulosa 
in  the  intact  adrenal.  Even  though  the  secretion  of  aldosterone  is  mode- 
rately stimulated  by  ACTH,  the  stimulation  is  not  maintained  in  spite  of 
continued  treatment  with  ACTH  (Liddle  et  al.,  1956),  and  the  response  is 
considerably  less  than  that  seen  following  changes  in  the  volume  of  extra- 
cellular fluid  or  in  body  potassium  (Bartter  et  al.,  1959).  The  glomerulosa 
will  respond  to  increased  ACTH  with  increased  secretion  of  aldosterone  for 
only  about  3  or  4  days,  and  then  the  rate  of  secretion  declines  in  spite  of 
continued  ACTH  and  reaches  base  levels  or  even  lower  levels  of  secretion 
in  about  a  week  (Liddle  et  al.,  1956).  After  this  period,  continued  ACTH 
will  not  increase  the  secretion  of  aldosterone  (Bartter  et  al.,  1959). Finally, 
the  secretion  of  aldosterone  is  only  slightly  depressed  by  suppressing  ACTH 
secretion  (Farrell  et  al.,  1955;  Liddle  et  al.,  1956;  Bartter,  1957)  or  by  hy- 

'  Since  completion  of  this  chapter,  there  has  been  marked  progresses  in  understanding 
the  regulation  of  aldosterone  secretion  in  response  to  hemodynamic  changes.  It  is  fairly 
certain  that  in  response  to  decreased  arterial  pressure  there  is  increased  relea.se  of  renin 
from  the  kidney.  The  end  product  of  this  release  is  angiotensin  II  which,  in  the  presence 
of  basal  levels  of  ACTH,  stimulates  aldosterone  secretion.  [For  a  review  see  J.  ().  Davis 
(1963).] 


2.  Endocrines  and  Populations  203 

pophysectomy  (Farrell  ei  al.,  1955).  These  results,  in  addition  to  those 
discussed  under  the  relationship  between  secretory  function  and  zonation 
of  the  adrenal  cortex,  clearly  indicate  that  the  secretion  of  aldosterone  is 
largely  independent  of  adrenocorticotropin  and  the  adenohypophysis  and 
that  its  responses  to  ACTH  may  reflect  the  increased  availability  of  aldoste- 
rone precursors  from  other  parts  of  the  cortex.  Nevertheless,  the  secretion 
of  aldosterone  in  dogs  appears  to  be  dependent  on  an  intact  pituitary  gland 
(Davis,  et  al.,  1959a).  However,  as  one  might  expect,  a  variety  of  stimuli 
which  produce  an  increase  in  the  secretion  of  ACTH  may  also  stimulate 
an  increase  in  the  secretion  of  aldosterone.  Farrell  (1958)  lists  position, 
surgery,  emotional  factors,  hypertension,  insulin  shock,  and  other  stimuli 
among  those  resulting  in  an  increased  secretion  of  aldosterone,  but  prob- 
ably none  of  these  are  without  an  effect  on  fluid  and  electrolyte  balances 
which  in  turn  would  effect  directly  the  mechanisms  regulating  the  secretion 
of  aldosterone.  On  the  other  hand,  there  is  a  marked  increase  in  the  secre- 
tion of  aldosterone  in  those  diseases  which  are  characterized  by  striking 
disturbances  in  fluid  and  electrolyte  metabolism,  such  as  congestive  heart 
failure,  hepatic  cirrhosis,  and  nephrosis  (Liddle  et  al,  1956) .  It  seems  likely 
that  the  increase  in  aldosterone  secretion  is  slight  in  those  circumstances 
which  produce  a  marked  increase  in  the  secretion  of  ACTH  and  of  the 
carbohydrate-active  corticoids  unless  there  is  also  involvement  of  fluid  and 
electrolyte  balances.  It  has  been  found  that  only  one,  the  A-1  fraction,  of  the 
several  distinct  fractions  of  ACTH  has  an  appreciable  effect  on  the  secretion 
of  aldosterone,  and  this  fraction  is  a  relatively  small  proportion  of  the  total 
amount  of  ACTH  which  may  be  secreted  (Farrell  et  al,  1958;  Farrell, 
1959a) . 

The  principal  regulation  of  aldosterone  secretion  seems  to  be  by  a  com- 
bination of  neural  and  neurohumoral  factors  in  response  to  changes  in  the 
volume  of  extracellular  fluid  or  body  potassium.  However,  there  can  be 
little  doubt  that  a  hormonal  factor  is  involved  in  aldosterone  secretion,  as 
recently  demonstrated  with  cross-circulation  experiments  by  Yankopoulos 
et  al  (1959) .  Recent  experiments  have  indicated  that  the  brain  may  secrete 
a  hormone,  glomerulotropin,  not  as  yet  isolated  and  characterized,  from  the 
region  of  the  pineal  body  which  stimulates  the  secretion  of  aldosterone 
from  the  adrenal  zona  glomerulosa  (Farrell,  1959a) . 

Small  changes  in  blood  volume  can  effect  striking  changes  in  the  rate  of 
secretion  of  aldosterone  (Bartter,  1957)  possibly  by  affecting  changes  in 
pulse  pressure  (Bartter  and  Gann,  1960).  Changes  in  blood  volume  elicit 
maximal  reciprocal  responses  in  the  secretion  of  aldosterone  and  it  appears 
that  this  system  is  the  most  sensitive,  as  well  as  the  most  important,  of 
those  involved  in  the  regulation  of  the  secretion  of  aldosterone  (Bartter, 
1957;  Bartter  et  al,  1959).  A  rise  in  blood  volume  reflexly  depresses  the 


204  /.  /.  Christian 

secretion  of  aldosterone  via  stretch  receptors  in  the  region  of  the  right 
striiim  or  adjacent  vena  cava  (Davis  ct  al.,  1956,  1957,  1958;  Liddle  ct  al., 
1956;  Bartter  et  al,  1958,  1959;  Farrell,  1959a;  Anderson  et  al,  1959),  the 
vagus  nerve  (Mills  et  al,  1958),  and  central  pathways  possibly  to  depress 
the  secretion  of  glomerulotropin  from  the  pineal  region  of  the  brain  al- 
through  Davis  et  al  (1959b)  indicated  that  the  vagus  is  not  involved  in  the 
afferent  pathways  of  this  control.  Conversely,  a  decrease  in  blood  volume 
stimulates  the  secretion  of  aldosterone  (Bartter  et  al,  1959),  although  the 
exact  pathways  and  mechanism  by  which  this  is  achieved  is  unknown. 
Bartter  and  Gann  (1960)  have  suggested  that  pulse  pressure  is  a  factor  in 
changes  in  blood  volume  which  affects  aldosterone  secretion.  A  drop  in 
pulse  pressure  stimulates  the  release  of  aldosterone  and  a  rise  inhibits  its 
release.  These  changes  apparently  come  about  through  changes  in  the  rate 
of  tonic  impulses  over  receptor  nerves  in  the  region  of  the  thyrocarotid 
artery. 

Another  system  that  regulates  the  secretion  of  aldosterone  involves  the 
levels  of  potassium  in  the  body.  A  deficiency  of  potassium,  therefore  a 
lowered  concentration  of  body  potassium,  results  in  a  lower  rate  of  secretion 
of  aldosterone  if  it  was  originally  elevated,  whereas  an  increase  in  body 
potassium  results  in  an  increase  in  the  secretion  of  aldosterone  (Bartter, 
1956;  Bartter  et  al,  1959).  A  rise  in  serum  potassium,  either  absolute  or 
relative  to  the  concentration  of  sodium,  is  associated  with  an  increase  in  the 
secretion  of  aldosterone,  but  it  is  not  known  whether  a  fall  m  potassium 
actively  inhibits  its  secretion  or  permits  it  to  return  to  base  levels  passively 
(Farrell,  1958) .  It  has  been  shown  that  these  changes  in  the  rate  of  secre- 
tion of  aldosterone  in  response  to  changes  in  body  potassium  are  inde- 
pendent of  sodium  concentration  in  the  serum  or  the  total  amount  of  so- 
dium in  the  body  and  are  also  independent  of  the  sodium:  potassium  ratio 
in  the  serum  (Bartter,  1956;  1957;  Bartter  et  al,  1959).  Similarly,  there  is 
no  evidence  to  suggest  that  altered  renal  hemodynamics  are  responsible  for 
the  altered  secretory  rates  of  aldosterone  (Bartter  et  al,  1956;  Cole,  1957). 
It  is  not  known  yet  whether  the  regulation  of  the  secretion  of  aldosterone  by 
the  body  potassium  is  directly  on  the  cells  of  the  adrenal  zona  glomerulosa 
or  is  mediated  through  central  channels  (Bartter,  1956;  Bartter  ct  al,  1959) . 
It  cannot  be  said  whether  serum  potassium,  intracellular  potassium,  or  a 
combination  of  both  effects  the  control  of  the  secretion  of  aldosterone,  but 
there  is  evidence  that  the  adrenal  cortical  cells  themselves  may  respond 
directly  to  this  type  of  stimulus  (Bartter,  1956) .  On  the  other  hand,  Farrell 
(1958)  suggests  that  the  effect  is  through  central  channels.  However, 
changes  in  potassium  are  not  as  important  in  the  regulation  of  the  secretion 
of  aldosterone  as  changes  in  the  volume  of  the  extracellular  fluid  (Bartter, 
1957;  Bartter  efaL,  1959). 


2.  Endocrines  and  Populations  205 

In  summary,  three  mechanisms  are  involved  in  regulating  the  secretion 
of  aldosterone.  The  first  and  most  important  regulating  factor  is  the  volume 
of  the  extracellular,  probably  intravascular,  fluid,  changes  in  which  act 
through  atrial  stretch  receptors  and  other  as  yet  unknown  pathways  to 
effect  reciprocal  changes  in  the  rate  of  secretion  of  aldosterone.  Decreased 
pulse  pressure  also  stimulates  increased  aldosterone  secretion  and  may  be 
one  way  in  which  changes  in  blood  volume  act.  Depression  of  the  secretion 
of  aldosterone  by  increases  in  blood  volume  requires  an  intact  vagus  nerve. 
The  second  mechanism  responds  to  changes  in  body  potassium;  a  rise  in 
potassium  resulting  in  elevation  of  the  rate  of  secretion  of  aldosterone  and  a 
fall  in  potassium  permit  the  secretion  of  aldosterone  to  fall  back  to  normal. 
Finally,  adrenocorticotropin,  or  at  least  a  fraction  thereof,  is  capable  of 
stimulating  the  secretion  of  aldosterone  in  the  intact  animal,  but  only  to  a 
moderate  degree  and  for  a  relatively  short  period  of  time,  although  the 
secretion  of  aldosterone  or  its  regulation  and  the  functional  integrity  of 
the  adrenal  zona  glomerulosa  apparently  do  not  depend  upon  adrenocorti- 
cotropin. Glomerulotropin,  a  recently  described  hormone  from  the  pineal 
complex  region  of  the  brain  which  stimulates  the  secretion  of  aldosterone, 
may  be  an  important  link  in  the  regulating  system  depending  on  the  volume 
of  the  extracellular  fluid  or  body  potassium  or  both,  but  this  work  requires 
confirmation. 

The  actions  of  aldosterone  are  essential  in  combatting  incipient  shock 
mammals,  and  this  hormone  apparently  plays  a  vital  role  in  the  daily 
maintenance  of  fluid  and  electrolyte  homeostasis.  Aldosterone  also  may  be 
more  directly  responsible  for  maintaining  blood  pressure  and  counteracting 
hemoconcentration  through  its  activity  in  correcting  alterations  in  blood 
volume. 

h.  The  zona  fasciculata.  (1)  The  hormones.  This  zone  of  the  adrenal 
cortex  normally  secretes  hydrocortisone  (Kendall's  compound  F),  corti- 
costerone  (Kendall's  compound  B),  small  amounts  of  cortisone  (Kendall's 
compound  E),  11-deoxycorticosterone  (Kendall's  compound  A),  11-de- 
oxycorticosterone  (DOC,  DCA,  or  DOCA),  ll-deoxy-17-hydrocorti- 
costerone  (Reichstein's  compound  S),  and  C19  ketosteroids,  usually  andro- 
genic, the  amounts  and  proportions  depending  on  the  species  and  the  circum- 
stances. Although  modification  of  this  concept  is  required  in  the  light  of  the 
work  of  Symington  and  his  co-Avorkers  (1958)  (c/.  above).  Their  experi- 
ments indicate  that  the  reticularis  is  the  part  of  the  cortex  that  normally 
produces  corticoids  and  17-ketosteroids  at  rest,  and  that  the  fasciculata  be- 
comes functional  with  increased  stimulation.  In  other  words,  the  reticularis 
is  the  active  part  of  the  cortex  and  the  fasciculata  is  a  resting  portion. 
Actually  this  work  indicates  that  the  morphologic  separation  of  the  cortex 
into  fasciculata  and  reticularis  is  unjustified.  In  addition  to  aldosterone,  the 


206  /•  /•  Christian 

normally  important  adrenocortical  hormones  are  corticosterone  and  hydro- 
cortisone, and  their  respective  ratios  vary  from  species  to  species  (Bush, 
1953;  Nelson,  1955)  and  possibly  with  the  degree  of  cortical  stimulation 
(Bradlow  and  Gallagher,  1957).  The  ratio  of  hydrocortisone  to  corticoste- 
rone (F:B  ratio)  may  vary  from  less  than  0.05  in  rats  and  rabbits  to  greater 
than  20  in  monkeys  (Bush,  1953;  Reif  and  Longwell,  1958;  Dorfman, 
1959) .  Most  species  lie  between  these  two  extremes  (Bush,  1953) .  However, 
there  is  little  doubt  that  in  most  species  studied,  exclusive  of  rats  and  mice, 
these  two  steroids  form  from  80%  to  95%  of  the  total  adrenal  secretion  of 
corticoids  (Jones,  1957).  Corticosterone  is  the  principal  carbohydrate - 
active  corticoid  secreted  by  mice,  rats  and,  rabbits  (Bush,  1953;  Hofmann, 
1956;  1957;  Reif  and  Longwell,  1958;  Wilson  et  al,  Bloch  and  Cohen,  1960) . 
whereas  hydrocortisone  is  the  principal  corticoid  in  other  species,  including 
guinea  pigs,  hamsters,  ferrets,  cats,  monkeys,  sheep,  and  humans  (Bush, 
1953;  Nelson,  1955;  Jones,  1957;  Peron  and  Dorfman,  1958,  Schindler  and 
Knigge,  1959a,  b).  The  adrenals  of  house  mice  and  rats  apparently  secrete 
large  amounts  of  1  l-hydroxy-**-androstene-3 ,  17-dione  ( 1 1-0H4AD)  1 1-hy- 
droxyandrostene-3,17-dione,  and  other  closely  related  steroids  as  major 
components  of  their  natural  adrenal  secretory  product  in  addition  to 
corticosterone  and  very  small  amounts  of  hydrocortisone  and  other  corti- 
coids (Sweat  and  Farrell,  1952;  Bush,  1953;  Hofmann,  1956;  Bahn  et  al., 
1957;  Poore  and  Hollander  1957;  Wilson  et  al.,  1958  Bloch  and  Cohen 
1960).  Probably  all  species  secrete  11-0H4AD  and  closely  related  Cig 
steroids,  but  usually  in  proportionately  small  amounts  (Bradlow  and  Gal- 
lagher, 1957;  Gallagher,  1958).  However,  it  has  been  shown  recently  that 
the  adrenal  androgen,  dehydroepiandrosterone,  comprises  about  50%  of 
the  total  secretion  of  steroids  by  the  human  adrenal  cortex  (Vande  Wiele 
and  Lieberman,  1960) .  The  general  problem  of  the  secretion  of  sex  steroids 
by  the  adrenal  cortex  has  not  been  studied  until  recently  in  the  same 
detail  as  the  carbohydrate-active  corticoids,  especially  with  regard  to 
differences  among  species,  but  there  is  no  doubt  that  they  are  secreted  b}^ 
the  cortex  (Gallagher,  1958) .  These  Cig  steroids  may  be  normal  products, 
metabolites,  or  intermediate  metabolites  in  the  synthesis  of  other  steroids 
(Dorfman  and  Shipley,  1956;  Gallagher,  1958).  Apparently  there  is  con- 
siderable variation  with  species  with  respect  to  the  secretion  of  androgens 
and  androgen  precursors  (Bush,  1953;  Jones,  1957;  Gallagher,  1958;  Wilson 
et  al.,  1958) .  In  any  event,  the  adrenal  cortex  is  the  starting  point  of  C19 
steroids  which  may  act  as  weak  androgens  (Dorfman  and  Shipley,  1956; 
Gallagher,  1958) . 

Normally  cortisone,  hydrocortisone,  and  corticosterone  appear  in  the 
urine  as  metabolites  which  can  be  identified  and  related  to  the  parent  corti- 
coid by  the  appropriate  procedures   (Gallagher,   1958;  Dorfman,   1960). 


2.  Endocrines  and  Populations  207 

However,  there  are  other  metaboHtes  in  the  urine  in  smaller  (juantities 
which  cannot  be  related  specifically  to  particular  adrenocorticoids  without 
radioactive  labeling,  but  these  ordinarily  are  not  produced  in  appreciable 
ciuantities  (Gallagher,  1958).  The  types  and  quantities  of  the  metabolites 
of  a  particular  hormone  which  appear  in  the  urine  usually  provide  a  good 
index  of  adrenocortical  activity  for  relatively  longer  periods  of  time  than 
can  be  obtained  by  the  measurement  of  the  corticoids  in  the  plasma,  which 
only  reflect  the  immediate  situation  (Nelson,  1955).  However,  the  urinary 
metabolites  do  not  always  reflect  the  actual  adrenal  secretory  pattern,  as 
has  been  shown  for  mice  (Bradlow  et  at.  1954;  Wilson  ct  al.,  1958)  although 
in  many  instances  this  may  be  surmised  with  confidence  (Dorfman,  1960). 
In  summary  it  may  be  said  that  hydrocortisone  or  corticosterone  and  C19 
weak  androgens  are  the  major  secretory  components  of  the  adrenal  fascicu- 
lata,  but  that  other  carbohydrate-active  corticoids,  sodium-retaining  corti- 
coids, and  adrenal  androgens  are  also  secreted,  although  usually  not  in 
appreciable  quantities. 

It  is  impossible  to  make  hard  and  fast  statements  about  the  quantitative 
relationships  of  the  adrenocortical  hormones  to  one  another  because  of  a 
certain  degree  of  inherent  variability  and  because  the  techniciues  for  their 
measurement  are  not  sufficiently  refined  and  certain  for  such  detailed 
comparisons.  A  large  number  of  steroids  have  been  isolated  from  the  adre- 
nals of  various  species,  frequently  from  perfused  glands.  Some  of  these  may 
be  biochemical  artifacts,  but  many  are  probably  intermediate  products  in 
the  biosynthesis  of  the  normal  secretory  products,  or  possibly  steroids  which 
are  secreted  only  under  unusual  conditions  (Jones,  1957;  Bradlow  and  Gal- 
lagher, 1957;  Gallagher,  1958) .  It  is  not  known  whether  or  to  what  extent, 
some  of  these  steroids  are  secreted  naturally.  The  picture  is  complicated 
further  by  the  fact  that  the  liver  and  other  tissues  metabolize  the  steroid 
hormones  to  new  steroids  which  appear  in  the  circulation  and  urine  and 
which  may  have  biological  activity  to  varying  degree  (Gallagher,  1958). 
Therefore,  the  specific  roles  of  the  various  adrenocortical  steroids  and  their 
metabolites,  especially  those  that  appear  in  very  low  concentrations,  in  the 
economy  of  the  w^hole  mammal,  and  the  variations  in  their  secretory  pat- 
terns from  species  to  species  and  under  normal  and  abnormal  circumstances, 
needs  clarification.  Some  of  the  discrepancies  that  appear  in  the  literature 
regarding  the  relative  amounts  of  various  steroids  secreted  by  the  adrenals 
of  a  particular  species  seem  to  depend  on  whether  the  measurements  were 
made  in  vivo  or  on  perfusates  of  isolated  glands  (Bush,  1953;  Jones,  1957). 
It  seems  evident  that  appreciably  higher  proportions  of  steroids  which 
normally  are  secreted  in  low  concentrations  are  found  in  perfusates  than  in 
vivo.  However,  it  suffices  for  the  present  to  know  that  the  carbohydrate- 
active  corticoids,  hydrocortisone  and  corticosterone,  are  the  major  na- 


208  /.  /.  Christian 

turally  secreted  corticoids  of  the  zona  fasciculata  and  to  discuss  the  actions 
of  these  hormones  as  a  class. 

(2)  Adiojis  of  the  fascicular  hormones.  Hydrocortisone,  cortisone, 
and  corticosterone  have  important  effects  on  carbohydrate  metaboHsm 
and  therefore  are  classed  loosely  as  carbohydrate-active  corticoids.  They 
have  in  common  either  a  hydroxyl  group  or  ketonic  oxygen  at  the  carbon-11 
position,  and  those  with  the  most  pronounced  effects  on  carbohydrate 
metabolism,  hydrocortisone  and  cortisone,  have  a  hydroxyl  group  on  the 
C-17  of  the  steroid  nucleus.  Corticosterone  has  a  weaker  action  on  carbohy- 
drate metabolism  than  hydrocortisone  or  cortisone  (Dorfman,  1949;  Ingle, 
1950;  Parmer  et  al.,  1951;  Santisteban  and  Dougherty,  1954;  Dougherty 
and  Schneebeli,  1955;  Kass  et  al.,  1955;  Noble,  1955) ,  but  it  has  appreciably 
more  effect  on  salt-electrolyte  metabolism  than  either  of  the  others  (Noble, 
1955;  Farrell  et  al.,  1955;  Jones,  1957) .  Because  of  these  facts,  the  relatively 
small  amounts  of  hydrocortisone  which  are  normally  secreted  by  the  adre- 
nals of  mice  and  rats,  along  with  corticosterone,  have  been  held  responsible 
for  most  of  the  carbohydrate-active  corticoid  activity,  such  as  involution 
of  the  thymus,  which  has  been  observed  in  these  animals  (Wilson  etal., 
1958).  The  designation  "carbolwdrate-active  corticoids"  for  this  group  of 
steroids  by  no  means  reflects  all  their  activities.  These  corticoids  have  sup- 
pressive effects  to  varying  degrees  on  inflammation  and  therefore  are 
classified  also  as  anti-inflammatory  (antiphlogistic)  hormones  (Selye,  1950; 
Dougherty  1953) .  As  a  class  they  have  profound  effects  on  protein  metabo- 
lism, fat  metabolism,  growth,  oxygen  consumption,  and  a  number  of  other 
physiological  functions  (Noble,  1955;  Jones,  1957).  Hydrocortisone  and 
cortisone  are  the  most  powerful  of  the  fascicular  carbohydrate-active 
corticoids  and  corticosterone  the  least  powerful  with  respect  to  the  enume- 
rated activities  (cf.  above),  although  cortisone  is  not  produced  in  bio- 
logically important  quantities  in  any  of  the  species  so  far  investigated 
(Bush,  1953;  Nelson,  1955).  As  a  general  rule,  the  degree  of  activity  of  a 
corticoid  on  carbohydrate  metabolism  is  related  inversely  to  its  sodium-re- 
taining ability.  Finally,  it  should  be  noted  that  other  steroids  may  affect  the 
actions  of  the  corticoids;  for  example,  testosterone  and  estradiol  potentiate 
the  anti-inflammatory  action  of  the  carbohydrate-active  corticoids  (Tauben- 
haus,  1953),  and  testosterone  enhances  the  thymolytic  activity  of  cortisone 
(Selye,  1955;  Dorfman  and  Shipley,  1956) . 

The  carbohydrate-active  corticoids,  secreted  by  the  zona  fasciculata,  will 
maintain  life  in  adrenalectomized  mammals  (Ingle,  1950) ;  nevertheless  the 
exact  functions  of  the  adrenocortical  hormones  in  the  intact  normal  animal 
are  difficult  to  delineate  precisely,  as  these  hormones  are  integral  elements 
in  a  complex  system  of  endocrine  and  neural  responses  which  form  a  feed- 


2.  Endocrines  and  Populations  209 

back  system  to  maintain  homeostasis  or  to  meet  emergencies.  Nevertheless, 
much  has  been  learned  about  the  specific  activities  of  these  hormones  by 
the  classic  experimental  approaches  to  such  a  problem :  the  substitution  of 
pure  hormones  or  extracts  into  intact  and  adrenalectomized  animals  and 
refinements  of  these  procedures.  The  effects  of  injected  carbohydrate-active 
corticoids  are  closely  paralleled  by  those  produced  by  injecting  adreno- 
corticotropin  (ACTH),  the  hormonal  protein  of  the  anterior  pituitary 
responsible  for  stimulating  the  secretion  of  the  carbohydrate-active  and 
Ci9  steroids  from  the  adrenal  cortex  (Poore  and  Hollander,  1957;  Li  et  al., 
1957;  Lostroh  and  Li,  1957;  Wilson,  et  al.,  1958;  Farrell  et  al.,  1958). 

The  carbohydrate-active  corticoids  stimulate  gluconeogenesis  from  pro- 
teins, and  this  activity  is  reflected  by  hyperglycemia  and  glycosuria  (Ingle, 
1949;  1950;  Jones,  1957).  The  increased  levels  of  glucose  in  the  blood  and 
urine  are  also  partly  due  to  the  inhibition  of  glucose  utilization  (Jones, 
1957) .  These  hormones  also  increase  glycogen  deposition  in  the  liver  by 
accelerating  its  formation  and  depressing  its  release  (Ligle,  1950;  Jones, 
1957) .  Glycogen  deposition  commonly  is  used  to  bioassay  steroids  for  their 
gluconeogenic  activity  and  other  effects  of  carbohydrate  metabolism  (Dorf- 
man,  1949).  The  carbohydrate-active  steroids  not  only  increase  protein 
catabolism,  but  they  also  depress  protein  anabolism  (Engel,  1952).  These 
two  actions  on  protein  metabolism  are  reflected  by  an  increase  in  the  non- 
protein nitrogen  of  the  blood  as  well  as  an  increase  in  the  excretion  of  uri- 
nary nitrogen  (Selye,  1950) .  Lipogenesis  is  inhibited  by  the  carbohydrate- 
active  corticoids,  but  their  effects  on  lipid  metabolism  are  poorly  understood 
(Jones,  1957) .  There  is  considerable  evidence  to  indicate  that  hydrocorti- 
sone and  cortisone  increase  the  sensitivity  of  blood  vessels  to  the  actions  of 
epinephrine  and  norepinephrine,  and  that  these  steroids  perform  an  essen- 
tial function  in  maintaining  normal  tonus  of  the  vasculature  (Zweifach 
et  al.,  1953;  Ramey  and  Goldstein,  1957).  The  carbohydrate-active  hor- 
mones also  decrease  capillary  permeability  and  fragility  and  antagonize  the 
spreading  action  of  hyaluronidase,  presumably  by  their  effects  on  the 
ground  substance;  these  corticoids  appear  to  decrease  permeability  of  the 
ground  substance,  and  their  ability  to  decrease  capillary  permeability  may 
be  dependent  on  this  effect  (Seifter  et  al.,  1953;  Zweifach  et  al.,  1953).  In 
these  effects  the  carbohydrate-active  corticoids  are  opposed  by  the  actions 
of  the  sodium-retaining  corticoids  and  growth  hormone  (Seifter  et  al.,  1953; 
Kass  et  al.  1953b;  Dougherty  and  Schneebeli,  1955;  Kramer  et  al.,  1957) .  In 
addition  to  their  catabolic  effect  on  protein,  the  carbohydrate-active  corti- 
coids have  specific  suppressive  effects  on  osteogenesis,  chondrogenesis, 
mitosis,  growth  in  general,  connective  tissue  growth,  inflammation,  phago- 
cytosis, granulation,  and  antibody  formation  (Taubenhaus  and  Amromin, 
1950;  Baker,  1950,  Selye,  1951;  Dorfman,  1953;  Dougherty,  1953;  Tauben- 


210  /.  /.  Christian 

haus,  1953;  Bullough,  1955;  Dougherty  and  Schneebeli,  1955;  Kass  et  al., 
1955;  Irving,  1957) .  The  effects  on  inflammation  result  from  a  failure  of  the 
usual  inflammatory  cells,  lymphocytes  and  fibroblasts,  to  appear  at  the  site 
of  injury  (Dougherty,  1953;  Dougherty  and  Schneebeli,  1955).  The  lack  of 
an  adecjuate  inflammatory  response  together  with  the  failure  of  adetiuate 
granulation  to  take  place  markedly  delays  wound  healing  (Dougherty, 
1953;  Dougherty  and  Schneebeli,  1955) .  These  effects,  coupled  with  inhibi- 
tion of  phagocytosis  and  antibod}^  formation,  result  in  a  marked  decrease 
in  resistance  to  infections,  so  that  an  animal  may  be  rapidly  overwhelmed 
by  an  infection  (Thomas,  1953).  There  is  ample  experimental  evidence  to 
show  that  cortisone  and  hydrocortisone  decrease  host  resistance  to  infection 
by  a  wide  variety  of  pathogenic  viruses,  bacteria,  protozoan  and  metazoan 
parasites  (Thomas,  1953;  Shwartzman  and  Aronson,  1953;  LeMaistre^ia/., 
1953;  Kass  et  al.  1953b;  Robinson  and  Smith,  1953;  Whitney  and  Anigstein, 
1953;  Pollard  and  Wilson,  1955) .  Animals  resistant  to  particular  organisms 
may  be  made  nonresistant  by  these  steroids,  and  usually  mild  infections 
may  become  highly  virulent. 

High  physiological  doses  of  cortisone  or  hydrocortisone  in  the  pregnant 
mammal  may  result  in  the  development  of  malformations,  especially  cleft 
palate,  in  the  fetus,  the  particular  anomaly  apparently  depending  on  the 
stage  of  development  of  the  fetus  when  it  is  subjected  to  the  actions  of  the 
hormone  (Glaubach,  1952;  Fraser  et  al,  1953;  Davis  and  Plotz,  1954;  Kal- 
ter,  1954;  Moss,  1955).  Cortisone  and  hydrocortisone  both  produce  cleft 
palates  and  other  congenital  defects  in  the  fetus  when  injected  into  pregnant 
mice,  the  incidence  of  these  anomalies  being  greater  when  the  injections 
were  made  on  the  tenth  day  than  when  later  ( Fraser  et  al.,  1953) .  The  tera- 
togenic effects  of  cortisone  in  mice  have  been  shown  to  be  decreased  with 
increased  maternal  body  weight  and  to  be  affected  by  maternal  genotype 
(Kalter,  1954;  1956).  Treatment  of  pregnant  rats  with  high  physiologic 
doses  of  cortisone  results  in  a  significant  increase  in  intra-uterine  mortalit3\ 
occurring  minly  at  mid-term,  and  later  (Seifter  et  al.,  1951 ;  Davis  and  Plotz, 
1954) .  High  doses  of  cortisone  administered  to  nursing  mice  9-12  days  after 
parturition  depress  the  growth  of  progeny,  whereas  ACTH  and  low  doses 
of  cortisone  were  without  effect  on  the  offspring,  except  to  abolish  the 
difference  in  growth  rate  normally  seen  between  male  and  female  mice 
(Glaubach,  1952).  Cortisone,  and  to  a  lesser  degree  ACTH,  depresses  the 
growth  of  infant  rats,  stimulates  the  eruption  of  teeth,  opening  of  the  eyes, 
and  development  of  the  gingivae  (Parmer  et  al,  1951) .  Cortisone  in  a  total 
dose  of  0.5  mg.  given  to  newborn  rats  during  the  first  week  produced  long- 
term  damage,  as  indicated  by  the  failure  of  the  animals  to  attain  normal 
body  weight  after  three  months  (Parmer  et  al.,  1951).  Corticosterone  and 
pregneninolone  were  without  effect  in  these  experiments.  Cortisone  treat- 


2.  Endocrines  and  Populations  211 

mciit  of  neonatal  rats  can  also  result  in  marked  morphologic  changes  in 
the  brain  and  skull  (INIoss,  1955).  Cortisone  or  hydrocortisone  may  stimu- 
late lactation  (Selye,  1954),  but  the  mechanism  by  which  this  is  accom- 
plished is  unknown.  These  effects  cannot  be  attributed  to  an  inhibition  of 
the  secretion  of  gonadotropin  by  cortisone,  as  it  has  been  shown  that  even 
relatively  large  doses  of  cortisone  are  without  effect  on  the  production  of 
gonadotropins  (Byrnes  and  Shipley,  1950),  although  it  is  well  known  that 
enormous  doses  of  corticoids  do  exert  some  antigonadotrophic  activity. 

The  carbohydrate-active  corticoids  also  involute  lymphoid  tissues  by 
producing  degeneration  and  actual  fragmentation  of  the  lymphoid  cells, 
inhibition  of  differentiation,  and  depression  of  lymphocytopoiesis  (Selye, 
1950;  Dougherty,  1953;  Santisteban  and  Dougherty,  1954;  Gordon,  1955; 
Weaver,  1955).  These  effects  are  also  seen  following  injection  of  ACTH, 
with  an  increase  in  endogenous  corticosteroid  secretion  (Baker  ei  al.,  1951). 
Lj^mphocytolysis  evidently  serves  to  release  a  readily  available  store  of 
amino  acids  and  may  serve  to  provide  a  sudden  flood  of  stored  antibodies, 
which  are  normally  produced  m  the  lymphoid  tissues  (Keuning  et  al., 
1950;  Dougherty,  1953;  Kass  et  al.,  1953a;  Sundberg,  1955).  These  actions 
result  in  involution  of  the  thymus,  lymph  nodes,  and  malpighian  corpuscles 
of  the  spleen.  Therefore  weights  of  those  organs  may  provide  useful  indices 
of  adrenocortical  activity  when  they  are  used  along  with  other  indices  of 
adrenal  activity,  such  as  adrenal  weight,  and  appropriate  controls.  It 
should  be  remembered,  however,  that  androgens,  and  to  a  somewhat  lesser 
extent  estrogens,  are  capable  of  involuting  the  thymus  (Burrows,  1949; 
Weaver,  1955) ;  therefore  cognizance  must  be  taken  of  this  fact  when  using 
thymic  involution  as  a  means  of  appraising  adrenocortical  activity.  How- 
ever, the  lymph  nodes  lose  weight  only  after  treatment  of  the  animal  with 
ACTH  or  carbohydrate-active  corticoids  (Weaver,  1955).  Estrogen,  tes- 
tosterone, thyroid  extract,  adrenalectomy,  thyroidectomy,  and  gonadec- 
tomy  were  without  effect  on  the  lymph  nodes  in  these  experiments  (Weaver, 
1955) .  The  adrenal  carbohydrate-active  corticoids  also  depress  the  numbers 
of  circulating  eosinophils  and  lymphocytes,  so  that  counts  of  these  cells  are 
frequently  used  to  assess  the  functional  integrity  of  the  pituitary-adreno- 
cortical  system  (Speirs  and  Meyer,  1949;  1951;  Gordon,  1955;  Speirs, 
1955).  In  using  counts  of  eosinophils  or  lymphocytes  as  indices  of  adreno- 
cortical activity  in  wild  mammals  care  must  be  taken  (1)  to  standardize 
the  procedures  so  that  the  results  are  completely  comparable  from  count 
to  count,  and  (2)  not  to  elicit  an  adrenocortical  response  during  the  process 
of  handling  the  animal. 

The  biological  activity  of  ll^-hydroxyA^-androstene-3,17-dione  (llOH- 
4AD)  and  the  closely  related  steroid  11/3-hydroxytestosterone,  as  well  as 
other  related  C19  steroids,  deserve  further  comment,  as  one  or  the  other  of 


212  J.  J.  Christian 

thefirst  two,  probably  the  first,  is  a  major  secretory  product  of  the  cortex, 
presumably  of  the  zona  fasciculata-reticiilaris  of  house  mice,  rats,  and  very 
possibly  other  rodents  (Bush,  1953;  Wilson  ei  al.,  1958),  and  to  these  must 
be  added  dehydroepiandrosterone,  which  is  now  known  to  account  for  half 
of  the  steroid  product  of  the  adrenal  cortex  of  human  beings  (VandeWiele 
and  Lieberman,  1960) .  These  steroids,  as  a  group,  are  very  weak  androgens 
with  insufficient  activity  to  maintain  the  seminal  vesicles  and  ventral 
prostate  in  hypophysectomized  mice  (Bahn  et  al.,  1957),  although  they 
evidently  are  sufficiently  androgenic  to  produce  histologically  detectable 
changes  in  the  epithelium  of  these  organs,  if  not  in  changes  in  gross  weight 
(Davidson  and  Moon,  1936;  Lostroh  and  Li,  1957),  and  evidently,  if  they 
are  secreted  in  large  enough  ciuantities,  they  can  produce  masculinization 
in  humans  (Dorfman  and  Shipley,  1956).  In  addition,  Howard  (1959)  has 
shown  that  these  weak  androgens  are  more  strongly  androgenic  if  their 
activity  is  measured  in  terms  of  other  assays,  such  as  stimulation  of  the 
preputials  and  os  penis.  However,  the  C19  steroids  with  weakly  androgenic 
activity,  as  measured  by  their  ability  to  stimulate  growth  of  the  prostate 
or  capon  comb,  can  inhibit  the  secretion  of  gonadotropins  in  rats,  especially 
in  immature  animals  (author's  italics) ,  although  it  has  been  shown  that  the 
carbohydrate-active  corticoids  are  incapable  of  producing  this  effect 
(Byrnes  and  Shipley,  1950;  Byrnes  and  Meyer,  1951;  Wilson  et  al.,  1958). 
Therefore,  it  is  possible  that  an  increased  secretion  of  these  androgens 
steroids  by  the  adrenal  cortex  in  mice  and  rats  can  account,  at  least  in  part, 
for  the  suppression  of  reproduction  commonly  associated  with  circum- 
stances which  increase  the  secretion  of  ACTH  and  carbohydrate-active 
corticoids,  as  described  by  Selye  (1939). 

These,  in  brief,  are  the  actions  of  the  important  adrenal  cortical  hor- 
mones. Many  questions  remain  unanswered  regarding  the  functions  of  the 
cortical  hormones,  especially  wath  respect  to  their  relationships  to  each 
other  and  to  other  endocrines,  such  as  the  thyroid  and  pancreatic  islets. 
The  carbohydrate-active  corticoids  in  many  respects  are  antagonistic  to 
insulin  and  probably  suppress  thyroid  activity,  but  these  topics  will  not  be 
dealt  with  here.  The  actions  already  listed  are  the  major  activities  of  the 
cortex  which  will  enable  an  interpretation  to  be  made  of,  as  well  as  to 
anticipate,  the  results  in  other  species.  All  the  above  effects  have  been 
duplicated  by  injecting  adrenocorticotropin  into  intact  mammals  and 
thereby  stimulating  an  increased  secretion  of  endogenous  adrenocortical 
steroids.  They  also  have  been  produced  by  alarming  stimuli,  which  increase 
the  secretion  of  endogenous  ACTH  and  in  turn  endogenous  corticoids. 
Among  these  stimuli  are  cold,  emotional  trauma,  physical  trauma,  toxic 
agents,  and  many  others,  although  the  general  response  to  these  stimuli  is 
not  necessarily  quantitatively,  or  even  quantitatively,  similar  in  every  case. 


2.  Endocrines  and  Populations  213 

There  is  much  to  learn  about  the  differences  in  hormonal  response  to 
different  stimuli.  There  are  excellent  reasons  to  believe  that  the  various 
adrenocortical  hormones  are  not  always  secreted  in  any  fixed  proportion  to 
each  other  independently  of  the  nature  of  the  stimulus  or  its  degree  of 
severity  (cf.  above).  Therefore  it  seems  advantageous  to  speak  of  the 
responses  evoked  by  the  specific  stimulus  in  each  case  rather  than  of  a 
generalized  nonspecific  response  to  "stress." 

(3)  Regulation  of  secretion  of  the  fascicular  hormones.  The  regulation 
of  the  secretion  of  the  hormones  of  the  cortical  zona  fasciculata  has  been 
investigated  intensively,  and  a  great  deal  is  known  about  the  mechanisms 
involved,  although  many  questions  still  remain  unanswered.  It  is  well 
established  that  adrenocorticotropin  (ACTH),  a  protein  hormone  secreted 
by  the  anterior  pituitary,  stimulates  corticoid  production  and  release  from 
the  adrenal  fasciculata  and  to  a  lesser  extent  stimulates  the  secretion  of 
aldosterone  from  the  zona  glomerulosa  (Wexler  et  al.,  1955;  Allen,  1957; 
Farrell  ct  al,  1955;  Jones,  1957;  Li  et  al,  1957;  Farrell  et  al,  1958,  and 
other  references  cited  earlier) .  ACTH  also  stimulates  cellular  hyperplasia 
and  hypertrophy  of  the  cortex,  although  pituitary  growth  hormone  may 
play  an  important  role  in  stunulating  cortical  hypertrophy  (Cater  and 
Stack-Dunne,  1953,  1955;  Jones,  1957;  Lostroh  and  Li,  1958),  possibly 
acting  synergistically  with  ACTH  (Lostroh  and  Woodward,  1958).  How- 
ever, growth  hormone  has  no  effect  on  the  release  of  corticosteroids  (Rosen- 
feld  and  Bascom,  1956;  Guillemin  et  al,  1958) .  ACTH  also  has  a  number  of 
extra-adrenal  actions;  for  example,  it  stimulates  the  preputial  glands  and 
other  sex  accessories,  but  not  to  the  same  extent  as  does  testosterone 
(Davidson  and  Moon,  1936;  Davidson,  1937;  Jacot  and  Selye,  1951; 
Lostroh  and  Li,  1957) .  The  regulation  of  the  release  of  ACTH  from  the 
pituitary  is  not  clearly  understood,  although  there  no  longer  can  be  any 
doubt  that  for  the  most  part  a  neurohumor  from  the  hypothalamus  has  a 
major  role  in  regulating  the  production  and  release  of  ACTH  (IMcCann, 
1953;  McCann  and  Brobeck,  1954;  Harris,  1955a,  b;  Fortier,  1957;  Wood- 
bury, 1958).  The  regulation  of  the  release  of  ACTH  has  been  reviewed  by 
Harris  and  Fortier  (1954),  Harris  (1955b),  and  Fortier  (1956),  but  advances 
in  this  field  are  rapid,  and  much  additional  information  is  available  which  is 
not  included  in  these  reviews.  Some  doubt  has  been  cast  on  the  role  of  the 
hypothalamus  in  regulatmg  ACTH  secretion  in  response  to  surgical  trauma 
(Story  efai.,  1959). 

The  adenohypophysis  has  a  double  arterial  supply:  a  portal  system 
draming  from  the  hypothalamus  and  a  direct  systemic  arterial  supply 
(Green,  1951).  Branches  from  the  superior  hypophyseal  arteries  form  a 
tufted  plexus  of  capillaries  in  the  median  eminence  of  the  hypothalamus  in 


214  /.  /.   Christian 

intimate  association  with  nerve  fibers  of  the  supraopticohypophyseal  tract. 
These  vessels  drain  into  the  adenohypophysis,  where  they  break  up  to 
form  a  series  of  sinusoids.  Here  they  are  joined  by  capillaries  from  the 
systemic  arterial  supply.  The  direction  of  the  flow  is  from  the  median 
eminence  to  the  adenohypophysis.  Neural  fiber  tracts  from  hypothalamic 
nuclei  descend  through  the  median  eminence  and  give  off  branches  which 
terminate  in  intimate  association  with  the  arterial  portal  plexus  in  the 
median  eminence  (Scharrer  and  Scharrer,  1954).  The  remaining  fibers 
continue  on  their  way,  to  terminate  in  close  approximation  to  capillaries 
in  the  neurohypophysis.  Neurosecretory  material  is  believed  to  traverse 
the  sheaths  of  these  nerves  from  hypothalamic  secretory  nuclei  (Scharrer 
and  Scharrer,  1954;  Rennels  and  Drager,  1955).  There  is  evidence  that  a 
fraction  of  this  neurosecretory  material  is  responsible  for  stimulating  the 
release  of  ACTH  from  the  anterior  pituitary  (Drager,  1955;  Rothballer, 
1953;  Saffran  et  al.,  1955).  Evidently  the  material  is  released  by  the  appro- 
priate stimulus  from  the  terminations  of  the  neurosecretory  fibers  into  the 
pituitary  portal  vessels  and  is  then  borne,  via  the  portal  system,  to  the 
cells  of  the  adenohypophysis  proper  (Scharrer  and  Scharrer,  1954;  Rennels 
and  Drager,  1955).  It  is  generally  accepted  that  no  nerve  fibers  terminate 
in  the  adenohypophysis  and  that  there  are  no  direct  neural,  only  vascular, 
connections  between  the  adenohypophysis  and  neurohypophysis  (Green, 
1951).  The  identity  of  the  hypothalamic  corticotropin-releasing  factor 
(CRF)  has  not  been  determined,  although  it  appears  to  be  a  small  protein 
often  associated  with  Pitressin  (Saffran  et  al.,  1955;  Porter  and  Rumsfeld. 
1956).  One  group  of  investigators  believes  that  the  ACTH-releasing  ac- 
tivity is  associated  with  Pitressin,  in  particular  with  its  pressor  activity 
(ADH,  vasopressin)  (McCann,  1957;  jMcCann  and  Fruit,  1957).  ADH 
exerts  a  powerful  antidiuretic  action  on  the  kidney  at  many  times  the 
dilution  that  is  necessary  for  it  to  exert  a  pressor  effect  or  to  stimulate  the 
release  of  the  ACTH  (McCann  and  Fruit,  1957) .  It  has  been  suggested  for 
this  reason  that  the  concentration  of  ADH,  when  it  is  released  from  the 
posterior  pituitary  into  the  systemic  circulation,  is  sufficient  to  effect  anti- 
diuresis,  but  insufficient  to  stimulate  the  release  of  ACTH,  but  that  when 
it  is  released  directly  into  the  pituitary  portal  circulation,  it  reaches  the 
adenohypophysis  in  a  sufficient  concentration  to  effect  the  release  of  ACTH 
(McCann  and  Fruit,  1957).  It  should  be  noted  that  ADH  evidently  is 
released  simultaneously  into  the  pituitary  portal  system  from  the  hypothal- 
amus and  from  the  neurohypophysis  into  the  systemic  circulation  in  re- 
sponse to  stimulation. 

Nevertheless,  as  attractive  as  this  theory  may  seem,  good  evidence  is 
accumulating  from  a  number  of  sources  which  indicate's  that  the  corti- 
cothopin-releasing  factor  is  an  entity  separate  from  vasopressin,  although 


2.  Endocrines  and  Populations  215 

the  site  of  its  origin  in  the  hypothalamus  and  its  exact  identity  remain  mi- 
known  (Saffran  et  al.,  1955;  Porter  and  Jones,  1956;  Porter  and  Rumsfeld, 
1956;  Clayton  et  al.,  1957;  Schally  and  Guillemin,  1959),  and  that  the 
release  of  ADH  may  be  completely  unrelated  to  the  release  of  ACTH  (Mc- 
Donald et  al,  1957;  Schapiro  et  al,  1958) .  It  appears  that  this  substance  is  a 
small  protein  which  is  recoverable  from  hypophyseal  portal  blood,  from  the 
neurohypophysis,  or  from  extracts  of  appropriately  selected  portions  of 
the  hypothalamus,  and  that  it  is  probably  loosely  bound  to  and  travels  with 
a  much  larger  protein  which  is  inactive  with  regard  to  the  release  of  ACTH 
(Porter  and  Rumsfeld,  1956;  Guillemin  et  al,  1957;  Schally  and  Guillemin, 
1959).  The  activity  of  the  corticothopin-releasing  factor  may  be  augented 
by  the  simultaneous  action  of  epinephrine  (Saffran  et  al,  1955) .  As  is  so 
often  the  case,  the  solution  to  this  problem  may  prove  to  be  midway  be- 
tween, or  to  be  a  combination  of,  the  two  opposing  views.  It  is  possible 
that  there  are  hypothalamic  substances,  such  as  vasopressin,  other  than  a 
specific  corticotropin-releasing  factor  which  are  capable  of  effecting  the 
release  of  specific  ACTH  fractions  from  the  adenohypophysis.  Recent- 
evidence  indicates  that  different  stimulating  factors  are  involved  in  ac- 
tivating different  adrenocortical  functions  (Fortier,  1956;  Guillemin  et  al, 
1958 ;  Slusher,  1958 ;  Nowell,  1959) . 

In  spite  of  the  existing  uncertainty  with  regard  to  the  nature  of  the  speci- 
fic factor  or  factors  from  the  hypothalamus  which  stimulate  the  release  of 
ACTH,  there  is  little  doubt  that  hypothalamic  substances  carried  to  the 
adenohypophysis  by  the  portal  system  can  stimulate  the  release  of  ACTH 
in  response  to  alarming  stimuli,  although  Nowell  (1959)  has  suggested 
that  emotional  and  systemic  stress  involve  different  pathways  for  the 
release  of  ACTH.  However,  there  are  many  other  aspects  of  the  overall 
regulation  of  the  secretion  of  adrenocorticotropin  which  are  not  clear  at 
the  present  time.  One  of  these  aspects  is  the  control  of  the  normal  daily  low- 
level  secretion  of  ACTH.  There  probably  is  a  basal  release  rate  of  ACTH 
which  is  independent  of  the  hypothalamic-hypophyseal  portal  system  and 
which  is  sufficient  for  normal  maintenance  of  adrenocortical  function 
(Fortier,  1957) .  A  number  of  workers  maintain  that  the  release  of  ACTH  is 
subject  to  hypothalamic  control  only  in  response  to  acute  stressful  stimuli 
(Woodbury,  1958) .  The  level  of  circulating  corticoids  undoubtedly  exerts  a 
control  over  the  rate  of  secretion  of  ACTH,  as  circulating  adrenocorticoids 
are  capable  of  inhibiting  the  release  of  ACTH  under  most  circumstances, 
although  the  more  severe  the  stimulus,  the  greater  the  level  of  circulating 
corticoids  must  be  in  order  to  block  ACTH  secretion  (Sydnor  and  Sayers, 
1954;  Farrell  and  Laqueur,  1955;  Sydnor  et  al,  1955;  Richards  and  Pruitt, 
1957).  Fvu'thermore,  Fortier  (1959a,  b)  has  shown  that  the  corticotropin- 
releasing  effect  of  stress  is  influenced  markedly  by  the  level  of  circulating 


216  /•  /•  Christian 

corticoids.  Withdrawal  of  the  corticoids  enhances  both  the  release  and 
synthesis  of  ACTH,  its  predominant  effect  being  on  the  synthesis  (Fortier, 
1959a,  b) .  The  steroids  appear  to  act  on  the  pituitary  itself,  but  they  may 
act  also  on  the  hypothalamic  centers,  or  on  the  releasing  mechanism  or 
mechanisms  themselves,  or  even  may  act  to  some  extent  on  the  adrenal 
gland  directly  (Birmingham  and  Kurlents,  1958).  However,  the  corticoids 
evidently  are  not  capable  of  blocking  the  release  of  ACTH  in  response  to  all 
types  of  tissue  damage,  unless  the  steroids  are  present  in  very  large  quan- 
tities. It  has  been  suggested  in  the  case  of  tissue  damage,  such  as  burns, 
that  substances  are  released  from  the  site  of  the  injury  (Hume,  1953; 
Woodbury,  1958;  Share  and  Stadler,  1958),  which  are  able  to  stimulate 
directly  the  release  of  ACTH  by  direct  action  on  the  pituitary,  although 
final  proof  of  such  a  suggestion  is  lacking.  It  is  unlikely  that  histamine, 
epinephrine,  serotonin  or  a  variety  of  other  compounds  which  are  released 
during  tissue-breakdown  are  responsible  for  directly  stimulating  the  release 
of  ACTH  (Sandberg  et  al,  1953;  Guillemin,  1955;  Guillemin  et  al,  1957; 
Woodbury,  1958)  as  has  been  suggested.  Nervous  impulses  from  the  site 
of  injury  cannot  be  responsible  for  initiating  the  release  of  ACTH  following 
injury,  as  complete  denervation  of  the  part  or  transection  of  the  cord  fails 
to  block  the  release  of  ACTH  (Hume,  1953).  The  mechanism  by  which 
adrenocorticotropin  is  released  following  tissue  damage  needs  to  be  deter- 
mined. 

Finally,  there  is  evidence  that  two  or  more  substances  from  the  hypo- 
thalamus may  regulate  different  activities  of  the  adrenal  cortex.  Slusher 
(1958)  has  shown  that  lesions  in  one  region  of  the  hypothalamus  will  block 
the  usual  fall  in  adrenal  ascorbic  acid  which  follows  cortical  stimulation 
without  preventing  the  release  of  cortical  steroids.  On  the  other  hand, 
lesions  in  a  nearby  region  can  prevent  the  release  of  corticoids  without  pre- 
venting the  fall  in  ascorbic  acid.  Guillemin  ct  al,  (1958)  have  shown  that 
there  may  be  a  functional  separation  of  corticoid  secretion  from  ascorbic 
acid  depletion.  These  investigators  found  that  a  marked  secretion  of  corti- 
coids could  occur  in  the  absence  of  detectable  changes  in  adrenal  ascorbic 
acid  concentration  when  both  variables  were  measured  simultaneously  fol- 
lowing stimulation  by  ACTH  in  hypophysectomized  rats.  Finally,  there  is 
other  evidence  which  suggests  that  the  factors  controlling  cortical  hyper- 
trophy (adrenal  weight  factor:  AWF)  and  ascorbic  acid  (ascorbic  acid 
factor:  AAF)  reduction  are  separate  entities  and  may  respond  differentially 
to  different  stimuli  (Nowell,  1959) .  Nowell  (1959)  has  suggested  that  stim- 
ulation of  the  release  of  various  ACTH  factors  from  the  pituitary  may  in- 
volve different  mechanisms  for  different  stimuli;  for  example,  emotional 
stimuli  may  require  activation  of  the  hypothalamic  centers,  whereas  adre- 
nal regeneration  may  not  require  hypothalamic  activity. 


2.  Endocrines  and  Populations  217 

Plainly  there  are  many  problems  still  to  be  solved  before  a  complete  and 

detailed  explanation  of  the  regulation  of  the  release  of  ACTH  and  the 
fascicular  corticoids  will  be  possible,  but  the  evidence  for  the  mechanisms 
which  have  been  described  is  strong.  There  is  no  doubt  that  the  hypothala- 
mus has  a  major  role  in  the  regulation  of  ACTH  release  and  of  adenohy- 
pophyseal  function  in  general,  and  there  is  no  reason  to  question  the 
existence  of  such  a  neurohumoral  mechanism.  The  evidence  for  some  of  the 
details,  however,  is  less  conclusive.  The  cortical  glomerulosa  and  the  re- 
lease of  aldosterone  are  largely  independent  of  this  system  of  regulation,  as 
indicated  earlier,  except  for  a  very  moderate  response  to  ACTH  for  a 
limited  time. 

c.  The  junction  of  the  zona  reticularis.  The  zona  reticularis  has  been 
thought  to  secrete  sex  steroids  (Selye,  1947),  especially  androgens,  but 
the  evidence  for  such  a  function  is  poor  and  largely  inferential  (Dempsey, 
1948;  Creep  and  Deane,  1949b;  Jones,  1957).  This  zone  is  made  up  of  a 
continuation  of  the  cords  or  continuum,  as  the  case  may  be,  of  the  zona 
fasciculata,  the  cells  of  which  are  in  varying  degrees  of  activity  and  respond 
accordingly  to  stimulation  by  ACTH  (Jones,  1957).  By  and  large  they 
have  the  appearance  of  cells  of  the  fasciculata  which  are  declinmg  in  ac- 
tivity and  becoming  obsolete,  but  there  is  certainly  some  functional  capa- 
city in  the  reticular  cells,  as  indicated  by  various  histochemical  procedures 
and  the  presence  of  mitosis,  although  it  is  apparently  much  less  than  that 
of  the  zona  fasciculata  (Dempsey,  1948;  Creep  and  Deane,  1949b) .  Syming- 
ton et  al.  (1958)  have  presented  convincing  morphologic  and  histochemical 
evidence,  coupled  with  secretory  studies,  that  the  reticularis  normally 
secretes  the  hormones  usually  attributed  to  the  zona  fasciculata  and  that 
the  latter  is  in  a  resting  state  until  fvnther  stimulated  by  ACTH.  This  sug- 
gestion is  well  worth  considering  especially  as  morphologic  changes  are 
related  to  steroid  production  by  direct  measurement.  These  studies  necessi- 
tate a  revision  of  the  classic  concepts  of  the  functional  roles  of  the  reticularis 
and  fasciculata  which  have  been  based  only  on  morphologic  evidence. 

d.  The  problem  of  the  function  of  X-zone.  The  X-zone  of  the  adrenal  cortex 
of  the  immature  house  mouse  (Mus  musculus)  is  maintained  by  gonado- 
tropin rather  than  by  ACTH  or  growth  hormone  (Jones,  1949b,  1950,  1952, 
1957) .  Its  function  is  unknown,  and  histochemically  it  does  not  present  the 
appearance  of  being  a  secretory  zone  (McPhail,  1944;  Howard,  1939;  1946; 
McPhail  and  Read,  1942a,  b) .  The  cells  do  not  give  the  usual  histochemical 
reactions  for  steroids  or  other  lipids,  although  they  do  contain  ascorbic 
acid  (Jones,  1949a,  1950) .  There  is  no  evidence  that  the  X-zone  of  the 
house  mouse  secretes  sex  steroids  (McPhail,  1944;  Jones,  1957),  although 
such  a  function  has  been  postulated  by  several  investigators  (Jones,  1957) . 
In  fact  the  large  amounts  of  Cig  steroids  secreted  by  the  adrenals  of  mice  in 


218  ./•  ./•   Christian 

response  to  ACTH-secrcting  pituitary  tumors  were  from  the  cortictes  of 
adrenals  without  X-zones  (Bahn  et  al.,  1957).  On  the  other  hand,  Delost 
(1951)  suggests  that  the  X-zone  may  be  that  part  of  the  cortex  responsible 
for  secreting  the  adrenal  androgens  which  maintain  the  activity  of  the  vas 
deferens  and  ventral  prostate  of  meadow  voles  (Microtus  arvalis)  in  winter 
and  in  adrenalectomized  voles  (Delost,  1954).  The  evidence  for  this  sug- 
gestion is  tenuous  and  requires  further  investigation  and  confirmation.  The 
function  of  the  X-zone,  if  there  is  one,  remains  undiscovered  at  the  present 
time. 

3.  Adrenocortical-Gonadal  Interrelationships 

This  problem  has  been  discussed  by  Jones  (1957),  and  the  reader  is  re- 
ferred to  this  work  for  a  more  detailed  discussion  of  this  intriguing  problem. 

There  is  a  very  close  relationship  between  the  cells  of  the  adrenal  cortex 
and  those  of  the  gonadal  cells  with  respect  to  their  origin  and  function. 
Both  groups  of  cells  arise  from  the  embryonic  genital  ridge  of  the  coelomic 
mesothelium,  both  secrete  steroid  hormones  in  response  to  stimulation  by 
pituitary  hormones;  and  there  is  a  definite  overlap  in  their  functional 
capacities.  Even  though  the  adrenal  cortex  is  believed  to  be  a  source  of  sex 
steroids,  gonadectomy  in  normal  animals  is  followed  by  complete  involution 
of  the  sex  accessories  to  the  immature  condition  (Jones,  1957) .  The  involu- 
tion occurs  even  in  mammals  such  as  mice  and  rats  in  which  weakly  andro- 
genic Ci9  steroids  normally  are  an  appreciable  portion  of  the  cortical  secre- 
tory product  (Dorfman  and  Shipley,  1956;  Wilson  et  al,  1958).  Similarly, 
the  sex  accessories  and  secondary  sex  characteristics  which  are  under  hor- 
monal control  fail  to  develop  in  the  prepubertal  castrate  mammals  (Bur- 
rows, 1949;  Jones,  1957) .  Finally,  the  transplantation  of  the  adrenal  to  the 
uterus  does  not  alter  the  morphology  of  that  part  of  the  uterus  or  of  the 
vagina  which  is  in  direct  contact  with  the  graft  (Sakiz,  1956) .  These  facts 
cast  considerable  doubt  on  their  ability  of  the  normal  adrenal  cortex  of  any 
species  to  secrete  steroids  with  strongly  androgenic  or  estrogenic  actions. 
There  is  much  evidence,  however,  that  under  certain  circumstances  the 
adrenal  cortex  is  capable  of  assuming  sex  steroidal  activity,  for  example,  in 
the  androgenital  syndrome  and  similar  virilizing  conditions  in  man  (Dorf- 
man and  Shipley,  1956;  Gallagher  et  al,  1958).  A  striking  increase  in  the 
secretion  of  weakly  androgenic  adrenal  steroids  may  account  for  these 
effects  (Jones,  1957) ,  but  they  may  also  result  from  the  conversion  of  adre- 
nal androgens  into  testosterone  in  these  circumstances  (Dorfman,  1960). 
These  conditions  are  usually  associated  with  functional  tumors  of  the  cortex 
or  with  a  marked  increase  in  the  secretion  of  ACTH  with  an  accompanying 
cortical  hyperplasia,  but  without  an  appreciable  increase  in  the  secretion  of 


2.  Endocrines  and  Populations  219 

the  cortieoids,  although  with  a  marked  increase  in  the  secretion  of  C19 
steroids  (Dorfman  and  Shipley,  1956;  Bradlow  and  Gallagher,  1957,  Dorf- 
man,  19G0) .  It  is  also  possible  that  these  effects  arc  a  result  of  aberrant  bio- 
chemical pathways  in  the  synthesis  of  adrenal  steroids  which  produce 
greatly  increased  amounts  of  weakly  androgenic  steroids  or  possibly  some 
compounds  with  more  strongly  androgenic  activity  (Dorfman  and  Shipley, 
1956;  Bradlow  and  Gallagher,  1957;  Gallagher,  1958,  Dorfman,  1960). 
Finally  there  may  be  abnormal  or  greatly  increased  androgenic  metabolites 
of  adrenal  steroids  (Gallagher  et  at.,  1958).  It  is  well  known  that  gonadec- 
tomy  is  followed  by  a  marked  increase  in  the  production  of  gonadotropins ; 
therefore  if  the  production  of  sex  steroids  by  the  adrenal  cortex  normally 
was  controlled  by  gonadotropins,  one  would  expect  an  increased  secretion 
of  these  sex  steroids  to  follow  gonadectomy  and  to  be  reflected  by  con- 
tinued maintenance  of  the  sex  accessories.  This  is  not  the  case,  as  the  sex 
accessories  involute  nearly  completely  following  gonadectomy  (Burrows, 
1949;  Jones,  1957).  However,  it  has  been  shown  that  increased  secretion 
of  ACTH  is  followed  after  a  time  by  a  marked  increase  in  the  secretion  of 
Ci9  steroids  (Bush,  1953;  Dorfman  and  Shipley,  1956;  Bradlow  and  Gal- 
lagher, 1957;  Jones,  1957;  Wilson  et  al,  1958,  Vande  Wiele  and  Lieberman, 
1960).  Apparently  slight  stimulation  of  the  sex  accessories  by  adrenocorti- 
cal androgens  occurs  following  ACTH  treatment  of  gonadectomized  rats, 
but  these  effects  can  be  detected  only  histologically  (Davidson  and  Moon, 
1936;  Davidson,  1937;  Lostroh  and  Li,  1957,  Desclin,  1959) .  In  marked  con- 
trast to  the  general  tenor  of  these  results,  Delost  (1951)  reports  that  the 
adrenal  cortex  secretes  hormones  with  androgenic  actions  on  the  sex  acces- 
sories of  male  Microtus  arvalis,  as  determined  by  morphologic  criteria,  but 
this  work  has  not  been  confirmed.  Delost  (1956a)  maintains  that  cortisone 
will  stimulate  some  of  the  secondary  sex  organs  in  these  mammals,  but 
confirmation  of  these  res  ilts  also  is  needed  before  acceptance  of  his  conclu- 
sions is  warranted.  It  is  best  for  the  present  to  assume  that  the  adrenal 
cortex  of  most  species  normally  does  not  secrete  appreciable  amounts  of 
steroids  with  pronounced  androgenic  or  estrogenic  activity  and  that,  it 
usually  represents  abnormal  function  when  appreciable  quantities  of  hor- 
mones with  these  activities  are  secreted  by  the  cortex,  (Bradlow  and  Gal- 
lagher, 1957).  The  ability  of  the  adrenal  cortical  and  gonadal  endocrine 
cells  to  secrete  steroids  with  overlapping  activity  is  an  intriguing  problem, 
and  the  reader  is  referred  to  Dorfman  (1960)  for  a  more  detailed  discussion 
of  the  steroidal  biosynthetic  pathways  involved  and  the  elucidation  of  some 
of  these  paradoxes.  There  is  certainly  variation  between  species  in  the  types 
of  adrenal  steroids  normally  secreted.  Under  abnormal  conditions  there  is 
no  doubt  that  the  adrenals  produce  increased  amounts  of  steroids  which  may 
have  sex  steroidal  activity  and  that  the  ovaries,  for  example,  can  be  induced 


220  /.  J.  Christian 

to  secrete  steroids  with  cortical  or  androgenic  activity.  Hill  (1948)  has 
shown  that  under  the  proper  conditions,  the  ovaries  of  mice  can  assume 
adrenocortical  function  when  transplanted  to  the  ears.  Under  other  circum- 
stances the  ovaries  can  be  made  to  secrete  androgens  (Hill,  1937;  Delost, 
1955) .  It  must  be  apparent  that  the  whole  field  of  adrenal-gonadal  relation- 
ships is  poorly  understood,  especially  with  regard  to  species  differences. 

There  is  one  area  of  adrenal-gonadal  relationships,  however,  which  seems 
to  be  on  relatively  solid  ground.  Selye  (1939)  called  attention  to  the  fact 
that  a  variety  of  stimuli  which  elicited  increased  adrenocortical  activity 
also  inhibited  reproductive  activity.  Female  rats  subjected  to  a  variety  of 
alarming  stimuli  exhibited  ovarian  atrophy  and  more  or  less  permanent 
anestrus.  These  observations  have  been  confirmed  and  extended  for  a 
variety  of  species,  including  mice,  voles,  man,  and  others,  and  for  a  wide 
variety  of  stimuli  (Christian,  1959b).  The  entire  and  exact  mechanisms  by 
which  inhibition  of  reproductive  function  is  brought  about  are  not  known 
with  certainty.  It  may  be  due  in  part  to  a  direct  suppressive  action  of  the 
carbohydrate-active  corticoids  on  the  reproductive  organs;  for  example,  it 
has  been  shown  that  cortisone  and  hydrocortisone  depress  the  ovarian  re- 
sponse to  chorionic  gonadotropins  in  intact  rats  (Smith,  1955),  possibly 
by  direct  action  on  the  vasculature.  However,  it  is  more  likely  that  the 
suppression  results  primarily  from  an  inhibition  of  the  secretion  of  gonado- 
tropins from  the  anterior  pituitary  when  there  is  an  increased  production 
of  adrenocorticotropin.  Selye  (1939)  suggested  that  there  is  a  "shift"  in 
pituitary  function;  the  production  of  gonadotropins  is  sacrificed  in  order 
to  increase  the  production  of  "life-maintaining"  ACTH.  But  it  is  equally, 
if  not  more,  likely  that  the  secretion  of  gonadotropin  is  inhibited  by  in- 
creased amounts  of  cortical  androgens  following  stimulation  by  ACTH. 
The  ability  of  androstenedione  and  other  related  weakly  androgenic 
steroids  to  suppress  pituitary  gonadotropic  activity  has  already  been 
discussed  and  it  has  also  been  stated  that  these  steroids  appear  to  be  major 
natural  secretory  products  of  house  mice,  rats,  man  and  probably  other 
species.  It  is  particularly  interesting,  especially  to  those  studying  popula- 
tion phenomena,  that  immature  mice  are  significantly  more  susceptible 
than  mature  mice  to  the  suppressive  effects  of  steroids  on  the  secretion  of 
gonadotropins  (Byrnes  and  Shipley,  1950;  Byrnes  and  Meyer,  1951) .  There 
is  also  the  possibility  that  nervous  stimuli,  acting  directly  through  the 
hypothalamus,  can  depress  gonadotropin  secretion. 

Whether  one  or  all  of  these  mechanisms  is  involved,  there  is  a  decrease  in 
the  secretion  of  gonadotropin  with  the  result  that  the  secretion  of  sex 
steroids  by  the  gonads  is  inhibited  and  the  secondary  sex  organs  may  ap- 
proach the  appearance  seen  following  gonadectomy  or  hypophysectomy 
(Selye,   1939;  Christian,   1959b).  The  germinal  cells  of  the  gonads  also 


2.  Endocrines  and  Populations  221 

reflect  the  withdrawal  of  stimuhition  by  the  gonadotropins  and  sex  steroids. 
Inhibition  of  these  endocrine  functions  can  result  in  a  depression  of  all 
reproductive  activities,  including  the  onset  of  puberty,  lactation,  fertility, 
normal  mamtenance  of  the  embryos  in  utero,  size  of  the  secondary  sex 
organs,  and  other  functions  associated  with  full  functional  competence  of 
the  pituitary-gonadal  system  (Christian,  1956,  1959a,  b).  It  is  pertinent 
here  to  note  that  androgen  injected  into  female  mice  or  rats  under  10  days  of 
age  will  produce  permanent  sterility ;  this  finding  suggests  a  method  for  the 
production  of  sterility  or  markedly  delayed  maturation  in  natural  popula- 
tions (Barraclough,  1961;  Barraclough  and  Gorski,  1961).  Most  of  these 
effects  presumably  are  brought  about  by  a  decreased  secretion  of  gonado- 
tropins, but  some  of  the  failures  in  reproductive  function  in  these  situations 
undoubtedly  result  from  the  direct  effects  of  increased  levels  of  corticoids 
follow^ing  increased  secretion  of  ACTH.  Certainly  the  ability  of  the  carbo- 
hydrate-active corticoids  to  increase  protein  catabolism  and  suppress 
growth,  especially  of  the  connective  tissue  and  its  products,  and  to  suppress 
mitoses,  must  have  appreciable  effects  on  the  reproductive  process,  espe- 
cially on  the  developing  fetus,  as  we  have  indicated  earlier.  There  is  con- 
siderable evidence  to  indicate  that  alarming  stimuli  during  pregnancy,  pre- 
sumably with  a  marked  increase  in  the  secretion  of  adrenocortical  steroids, 
can  result  in  congenital  anomalies  dependent  on  the  stage  of  fetal  develop- 
ment (Fraser  et  al.,  1953;  Aycock  and  Ingalis,  1946;  Ingalls,  1956;  Curley 
and  Ingalls,  1957;  Ingalls  and  Philbrook,  1958). 

Finally  it  should  be  borne  in  mind  that  the  degree  of  reproductive  sup- 
pression will  vary  in  some  proportion  to  the  severity  of  the  inducing  stimu- 
lus and  that  there  w411  be  some  variation  with  species  with  respect  to  the 
particular  part  of  the  reproductive  cycle  which  will  be  most  severely  cur- 
tailed. For  example,  intra-uterine  mortality  and  resorption  of  the  embryos 
is  marked  in  house  mice,  but  apparently  is  inconsequential  in  Norway  rats, 
whereas  the  postparturitional  loss  of  young  is  as  great  or  greater  in  rats 
than  it  is  in  mice  (Christian,  1959b).  Voles  evidently  are  quite  susceptible 
to  depression  of  fertility  and  development  of  maturity  (Kalela,  1957) ,  but 
house  mice  are  affected  similarly  (Crowcroft  and  Rowe,  1957).  These 
variations  are  of  particular  importance  to  the  investigator  involved  in 
comparative  studies,  especially  in  studies  of  phenomena  relating  to  popula- 
tion density.  It  should  be  kept  in  mind  that  the  severity  and  duration  of 
the  stimulus  and  the  age  of  the  affected  mammal  also  will  have  an  important 
bearing  on  the  particular  stage  of  the  reproductive  cycle  affected  as  well  as 
the  degree  of  its  inhibition.  There  are  numerous  examples  of  modification 
of  reproductive  function  in  response  to  various  environmental  stimuli, 
probably  representative  of  activity  of  physiologic  adaptive  mechanisms. 
Baker  and  Ransom  (1932)  found  that  winter  temperatures  (5°C.)  signifi- 


222  /.  /.  Christian 

cantly  reduced  the  number  of  births  on  Microtus  agrestis  even  though  they 
were  maintained  on  summer  food.  Chitty  and  Austin  (1957)  have  called 
attention  to  the  effects  of  environmental  factors,  especially  social  "stress," 
on  changes  is  estrous  pattern  and  behavior  exhibited  by  Microtus  agrestis. 
Additional  examples  of  the  effects  of  deleterious  environmental  factors  on 
reproduction  will  be  given  later. 

There  is  another  aspect  of  the  suppression  of  reproduction  which  should 
be  mentioned  at  the  present  time,  although  it  is  not  directly  pertinent  to 
adrenocortical-gonadal  relationships.  Inanition,  starvation,  protein  defi- 
ciency, and  probably  other  dietary  inadequacies  are  capable  of  depressing 
or  totally  inhibiting  reproduction,  the  degree  depending  on  the  severity  of 
the  inadequacy  (Lutwak-]\Iann,  1958),  without  stimulating  increased 
secretion  of  ACTH  and  therefore  increased  adrenocortical  activity  (Baker, 
1952;  Christian,  1959b,  c;  Eisenstein,  1959),  and  adrenocortical  secretion  is 
actuallj^  depressed  in  rats  deficient  in  pantothenic  acid  (Eisenstein,  1957). 
The  experimental  evidence  on  which  this  statement  is  based  has  been 
derived  from  laboratory  mammals  and  therefore  does  not  necessarily  apply 
to  other  species  or  to  natural  populations,  but  the  burden  of  proof  lies  with 
those  who  claim  otherwise.  Recent  experiments  with  house  mice  have 
demonstrated  rather  conclusively  that  inanition  does  not  stimulate  the 
pituitary-adrenocortical  system,  although  its  effect  on  the  reproductive 
system  is  striking  ( Lutwak-Mann,  1958;  Christian,  1959b,  c).  Further- 
more, a  deficiency  of  vitamin  Be  or  partial  starvation  does  not  increase  the 
secretion  of  corticoids  nor  impau-  the  ability  of  the  adrenal  cortex  to  secrete 
them,  even  though  they  may  be  a  marked  cortical  hypertrophy  (Eisenstein, 
1959).  These  results  are  in  agreement  with  those  from  a  large  number  of 
earlier  experiments  (cf.  Baker,  1952;  Christian,  1959c) .  In  addition  Srebnik 
et  al.  (1958)  have  shown  that  a  protein  deficiency  results  in  a  failure  in  the 
secretion  of  gonadotropins,  and  jVIarrian  and  Parkes  (1929)  had  shown 
earlier  that  anterior  pituitary  extracts  could  correct  the  anestrus  produced 
by  a  dietary  insufficiency  of  vitamin  B  with  the  return  of  normal  estrus  and 
normal  changes  in  the  accessory  organs  of  reproduction  with  the  estrous 
cycle.  One  might  hypothesize  that  an  inadequate  diet  would  impair  the 
ability  of  the  anterior  pituitary  to  synthesize  its  protein  hormones.  How- 
ever, the  ability  of  the  pituitary-adrenocortical  system  of  mice  to  exhibit 
the  usual  responses  to  alarming  stimuli,  and  therefore  the  ability  to  elabo- 
rate adrenocorticotropin,  in  spite  of  inanition  indicates  that  this  is  probably 
not  the  case  (Christian,  1959c).  Therefore,  the  decreased  production  of 
gonadotropins  by  animals  subjected  to  inanition,  starvation,  or  other 
dietary  inadequacies  probably  operates  through  an  unknown  mechanism, 
probably  at  the  hypothalamic  level,  which  does  not  reflect  an  inability  of 
the  anterior  pituitary  to  synthesize  protein  hormones. 


2.  Endocrines  and  Populations  223 

4.  Epinephrine  and  Norepinephrine ;  the  Hormones  of  the  Aduexal 
Medulla  and  Sympathetic  Nervous  System 

The  adrenal  medulla  and  its  hormones,  epinephrine  and  norepinephrine, 
have  been  the  subjects  of  numerous  and  ^"oluminous  reviews  and  are  also 
discussed  in  considerable  detail  in  most  good  texts  of  physiology  and  phar- 
macology. Therefore,  except  for  a  few  aspects,  a  detailed  account  of  these 
hormones  and  their  physiologic  and  pharmacologic  actions  will  not  be  given 
here.  The  reviews  of  the  following  investigators  may  be  referred  to  for  a 
more  detailed  coverage  of  the  subject:  Hartman  and  Brownell,  1949;  von 
Euler,  1951;  Hagen  and  Welch,  1956;  Gaddum  and  Holzbauer,  1957; 
Ramey  and  Goldstein,  1957;  Elmadjian  et  al.,  1958. 

The  adrenal  medulla  is  an  integral  part  of  the  sympathetic  nervous  sys- 
tem. The  medulla  is  homologous  with  the  sympathetic  ganglia  and  is  in- 
nervated by  cholinergic  preganglionic  fibers  of  the  splanchnic  sympathetic 
nerves.  Upon  stimulation  of  the  sympathetic  nervous  system,  the  adrenal 
medulla  discharges  norepinephrine  and  epinephrine  into  the  systemic  circu- 
lation, the  proportions  of  these  two  compounds  varying  with  the  species 
and  with  the  nature  of  the  stimulus  (von  Euler,  1951;  Hagen  and  Welch, 
1956;  Gaddum  and  Holzbauer,  1957;  Gray  and  Beetham,  1957;  Elmadjian 
et  al.,  1958;  Goldfien  et  al.,  1958).  Norepinephrine  is  also  secreted  by  the 
postganglionic  sympathetic  nerves  and  by  the  extra-adrenal  chromaffin 
tissue  of  the  sympathetic  nervous  system  (von  Euler,  1951;  Hagen  and 
Welch,  1956).  Norepinephrine  probably  is  the  neurohumoral  transmitter 
substance  of  the  postganglionic  sympathetic  nervous  system,  and  appar- 
ently is  released  on  nervous  stimulation  at  the  sympathetic  nerve  endings 
(von  Euler,  1951 ;  Hagen  and  Welch,  1956;  Richardson  and  Woods,  1959). 
These  two  hormones  have  profound  effects  on  the  circulatory  system  and 
glucose  and  fat  metabolism,  but  by  and  large  their  effects  are  short  lived, 
owing  to  their  rapid  destruction  in  the  body  by  the  cytochrome  oxidase 
system  or  by  amine  oxidases  (Bell  et  al.,  1950;  Gaddum  and  Holzbauer, 
1957).  Norepineplirine  and  epinephrine  have  a  variety  of  effects  over  the 
entire  body  which  are  brought  about  largely  by  their  actions  on  smooth 
muscle  and  which  in  general  parallel  the  effects  of  stimulating  the  sympathe- 
tic nervous  system  (Hartman  and  Brownell,  1949). 

Epinephrine  and  norepinephrine  both  have  profound  pressor  effects  on 
the  cardiovascular  system  and  on  the  levels  of  blood  sugar;  but  epinephrine 
in  general  has  a  greater  effect  on  carbohydrate  metabolism  and  produces  a 
greater  hyperglycemic  response  than  norepinephrine,  whereas  norepineph- 
rine has  a  greater  pressor  effect  than  epinephrine  (Gaddum  and  Holzbauer, 
1957).  In  many  ways  the  actions  of  these  two  hormones  are  similar,  but  in 
others  they  have  opposing  actions.  Norepinephrine  in  general  produces  a 


224  /•  /.  Christian 

greater  rise  in  blood  pressure  than  epinephrine  because  it  increases  overall 
peripheral  resistance,  largely  by  constricting  the  vasculature  of  the  muscles 
as  well  as  of  the  skin  (Bell  et  al.,  1950) .  Epinephrine  produces  a  greater 
constriction  of  the  vasculature  of  the  skin,  but  dilates  the  vessels  of  the 
skeletal  muscles  and  increases  the  cardiac  output  by  increasing  the  rate  and 
strength  of  the  heart  beat.  The  effects  of  norepinephrine  on  cardiac  output 
are  variable.  Both  of  these  amines  decrease  the  formation  of  urine,  produce 
relaxation  of  the  gut  by  inhibition  of  its  smooth  muscle,  produce  splenic 
contraction,  dilate  the  bronchi,  inhibit  the  bladder,  and  produce  pupillary 
dilatation  (Bell  et  al.,  19501 .  Both  produce  a  rise  in  blood  sugar  but,  as  we 
have  mentioned,  epinephrine  produces  a  greater  rise  than  norepinephrine. 
The  rise  in  blood  sugar  and  subsequent  glucosuria  result  from  the  mobiliza- 
tion of  glucose  from  the  readily  available  stores  in  the  liver,  and  secondarily 
from  the  muscles.  The  immediate  effect  of  epinephrine  is  to  release  glucose 
from  the  available  stores  of  liver  glycogen ;  therefore  the  magnitude  of  the 
resulting  hyperglycemia  depends  on  the  amount  of  glycogen  in  the  liver 
(Hartman  and  Brownell,  (1949).  The  eventual  effect  of  epinephrine,  after 
an  initial  depletion  of  liver  and  muscle  glycogen,  is  to  shift  carbohydrate 
from  the  muscles  to  the  liver,  as  the  uptake  of  glucose  by  muscle  is  de- 
pressed and  it  is  well  known  that  lactic  acid  derived  from  muscle  glycogen 
is  used  by  the  liver  to  synthesize  glycogen. 

The  actions  of  these  hormones  are  the  classic  preparations  for  "fight  or 
flight"  in  response  to  emergency  situations  (Cannon,  1915, 1932) .  The  com- 
bined activity  of  epinephrine  and  norepinephrine  ensure  adequate  blood 
and  glucose  to  the  muscles,  increased  oxygenation,  and  adequate  blood 
flow.  An  increased  supply  of  oxygen  to  the  tissues  is  ensured  by  an  increase 
in  respiratory  rate,  bronchial  dilatation,  and  contraction  of  the  splenic 
capsule  with  release  of  stored  red  blood  cells  into  the  circulation.  Other 
activities,  unneeded  in  an  emergency,  are  suppressed.  The  adrenal  medulla 
and  sympathetic  nervous  system  respond  to  cold,  fear,  rage,  trauma,  pain, 
blood  loss,  anoxia,  emotional  tension,  and  a  variety  of  additional  alarming 
stimuli.  A  variety  of  chemical  agents,  such  as  potassium  and  serotonin,  will 
release  the  catechol  amines  from  the  medulla  (Gaddum  and  Holzbauer, 
1957).  The  sympathico-adrenal  system  represents  a  major  and  immediate 
reaction  system  of  the  body  to  prepare  for,  or  to  counteract  the  effects  of, 
an  emergency  situation.  The  acute  response  is  relatively  short  lived  and 
serves  to  maintain  life  and  counteract  shock  until  the  emergency  passes  or 
until  longer-acting  adaptive  systems,  such  as  the  pituitary-adrenocortical 
system,  take  over  and  aid  in  physiologic  adaptation  to  the  situation. 

Recent  evidence  has  shown  that  norepinephrme  is  normally  found  in  the 
walls  of  the  arteries  (Schmiterlow,  1948),  and  that  it  plays  a  major,  per- 
haps decisive  role  in  the  maintenance  of  normal  vascular  tonus  and  reac- 


2.  Endocrines  and  Populations  225 

tivity,  achieving  the  latter  by  diminishing  the  sensitivity  of  the  arterial 
musculature  to  epinephrine  and  norepinephrine  by  maintaining  a  constant 
low  level  of  pressor  amines  in  the  arterial  wall  (Burn  and  Rand,  1958a,  b). 
The  source  of  the  norepinephrine  in  the  arterial  walls  is  apparently  the 
chromaffin  tissue  including  the  adrenal  medulla,  or  sympathetic  neural 
terminations  which  appear  to  release  a  low  level  of  these  cathechols  amines 
constantly  into  the  circulation  (Bell  et  al.,  1950;  Gaddum  and  Holzbauer, 
1957).  Experiments  with  reserpine  (Burn  and  Rand,  1958a,  b;  Eranko 
and  Hopsu,  1958) ,  which  depletes  the  epinephrine  and  norepinephrine  from 
the  adrenal  medulla  and  sympathetic  chromaffin  tissue,  depletes  the  con- 
tent of  catechol  pressor  amines  from  the  arterial  walls  and  thereby  makes 
them  excessively  sensitive  to  circulating  epinephrine  and  norepinephrine. 
However,  the  arteries  are  unresponsive  to  other  noncatechol  pressor  amines 
which  apparently  exert  their  usual  effects  by  releasing  the  norepinephrine  in 
the  arterial  walls  (Burn  and  Rand,  1958a,  b).  There  is  also  evidence  that 
the  adrenal  carbohydrate-active  corticoids  have  a  part  m  the  maintenance 
of  arterial  tonus  and  reactivity  by  increasing  the  sensitivity  of  the  vascula- 
ture to  the  action  of  epinephruie  and  norepinephrine  (Ramey  and  Gold- 
stein, 1957) . 

Reserpine  is  a  pharmacologic  agent  which  causes  the  disappearance  of 
the  catechol  pressor  amines  from  the  chromaffin  tissue  and  subsequently 
from  the  arteries  (Burn  and  Rand,  1957,  1958b),  but  stimulation  of  the 
sympathetic  nervous  system  also  can  exhaust  the  pressor  amines  from  the 
sympathetic  ganglia  and  adrenal  medulla,  although  the  stimulus  must 
persist  for  30  minutes  or  longer  to  achieve  exhaustion  of  the  adrenal  medul- 
las of  dogs  (Gaddum  and  Holzbauer,  1957).  Therefore,  it  is  conceivable 
that  prolonged  and  intense  emotional  stimuli,  such  as  one  might  expect  as 
a  result  of  social  interactions  between  animals  in  populations  of  excessive 
density,  might  exhaust  the  stores  of  pressor  amines,  especially  in  the  sub- 
ordinate animals.  If  such  an  event  occurs,  one  might  anticipate  that  there 
would  be  a  subsequent  depletion  of  the  arterial  norepinephrine  and  loss  of 
arterial  tonus  which  might  account  for  the  occasional  deaths  due  to  the 
shock  seen  in  mice  shortly  after  they  are  first  placed  together  (Christian, 
1955b)  or  following  more  protracted  periods  of  social  strife  (Frank,  1953). 
A  loss  of  vascular  tonus  with  a  subsequent  hypotension,  and  eventually 
shock  with  circulatory  collapse,  could  explain  the  symptoms  observed  by 
Frank  (1953)  in  dense  populations  of  Microtus  in  the  wild  or  in  captivity 
or  might  be  a  part  of  the  picture  of  "shock  disease"  (Green  and  Larson, 
1938;  Green  et  al,  1939;  Christian  and  Ratcliffe,  1952).  There  is  also  the 
possibility  of  a  simultaneous  exhaustion  of  readily  available  glucose  re- 
serves by  the  action  of  epinephrine,  especially  in  animals  with  a  high 
metabolic  rate,  or,  perhaps  more  likely,  a  loss  of  the  ability  to  mobilize 


226  /.  /.   Christian 

reserves  due  to  exhaustion  of  the  supplies  of  epinephrine.  Such  a  mecha- 
nism, albeit  conjectural,  may  provide  a  better  explanation  for  the  immedi- 
ate and  precipitate  cause,  the  proximate  cause,  of  "shock  disease"  than  the 
previously  postulated  pituitary- adrenocortical  exhaustion  (Christian, 
1950b) ,  although  adrenocortical  hyperactivity  probably  plays  an  additive 
or  even  synergistic  role  in  the  cause  of  the  immediate  mortality  in  "shock 
disease."  These  conjectures  are  not  meant  to  relegate  the  pituitary-adreno- 
cortical-gonadal  system  to  a  secondary  role  in  the  more  prolonged  and 
chronic  effects  of  increased  population  density  or  in  the  control  of  popula- 
tion growth,  as  we  shall  see  later.  However,  the  available  experimental 
evidence  places  the  sympathico-adrenal  medullary  system  in  the  forefront 
of  the  mechanisms  w^hich  respond  acutely  and  which  need  investigation  in 
relation  to  "shock  disease"  and  the  sudden  and  mass  mortality  associated 
therewith,  as  well  as  in  relation  to  those  sudden  deaths,  resembling  hypo- 
glycemic shock,  which  occur  on  first  placing  strange  mammals  together. 

The  development  of  techniques  to  measure  the  secretion  of  the  catechol 
amines  has  led  to  a  number  of  investigations  on  the  secretion  of  epinephrine 
and  norepinephrine  in  response  to  a  variety  of  stimuli.  One  can  almost  pre- 
dict which  of  these  two  amines  will  be  secreted  in  response  to  a  particular 
stimulus  by  knowing  which  has  the  greater  effect  on  blood  sugar  or  on  blood 
pressure.  Norepinephrine  appears  to  be  released  preferentially  by  the  adre- 
nal medulla  during  rest  (Gaddum  and  Holzbauer,  1957).  The  plasma 
concentration  of  norepinephrine  rises  sharply  with  acute  muscular  work, 
but  the  response  of  epinephrine  varies  from  no  change  to  a  marked  rise, 
depending  on  the  individual  (Gray  and  Beetham,  1957).  Both  return  to 
normal  levels  within  15  to  30  minutes  after  cessation  of  work.  Hypoglyce- 
mia is  followed  by  a  marked  and  sharp  rise  in  the  medullary  secretion  of 
epinephrine  with  a  much  less  marked  rise  in  norepinephrine  (Gaddum  and 
Holzbauer,  1957;  Goldfien  ct  al.,  1958).  Infusion  of  glucose  promptly  re- 
turns their  secretion  to  normal  levels.  Repeated  production  of  hypoglyce- 
mia with  insulin  eventually  leads  to  a  decline  in  the  secretion  of  epinephrine, 
evidently  due  to  medullary  exhaustion  (Elmadjian  ct  al.,  1958).  Hypoten- 
sion produces  a  marked  rise  in  the  secretion  of  norepinephrine,  but  little  or 
no  rise  in  epinephrine  (Elmadjian  et  al.,  1958).  Surgical  shock  or  a  change 
in  position  from  recumbent  to  standing  leads  to  a  sharp  rise  in  the  secretion 
of  norepinephrine  with  or  without  a  rise  in  epinephrine.  (Elmadjian  et  al., 
1958).  Tense,  anticipatory  but  passive  emotional  situations  produce  a 
marked  rise  in  the  secretion  of  epinephrine,  norepinephrine  being  secreted 
in  normal  amounts,  but  active,  aggressive  emotional  situations  are  related 
to  a  rise  in  norepinephrine  (Elmadjian  et  al.,  1958) .  If  the  emotional  display 
is  intense  enough,  both  epinephrine  and  norepinephrine  are  elevated.  It  is 
of  particular  interest  that  in  adrenalectomized  patients  the  secretion  levels 
of  norepinephrine  and  their  diurnal  variations  are  completely  normal. 


2.  Endocrines  and  Populations  227 

indicating  that  the  normal  daily  secretion  of  norepinephrine  is  largely  from 
extra-adrenal  chromaffin  tissues,  whereas  the  secretion  of  epinephrine  is 
largely  from  the  adrenal  medulla.  These  facts  are  particularly  pertinent  to 
investigations  of  the  physiologic  responses  of  mammals  to  sociopsychologic 
factors. 

The  secretion  of  epinephrine  and  norepinephrine  evidently  is  controlled 
by  separate  hypothalamic  centers  (Elmadjian  et  al.,  1958).  The  secretion 
of  norepinephrine  apparently  is  related  to  an  area  in  the  posterior  hypothal- 
amus and  that  of  epinephrine  to  a  lateral  area.  However,  the  vasomotor 
center  is  in  the  region  of  the  floor  of  the  fourth  ventricle  in  the  medulla 
oblongata,  and  it  is  the  most  sensitive  area  relating  to  the  secretion  of 
epinephrine  (Elmadjian  d  o/.,  1958). 

In  addition  to  the  actions  of  epinephrine  and  norepinephrine  listed 
above,  these  compounds  of  the  sympathico-adrenal  system  have  important 
activity  relationships  with  other  endocrine  organs  and  their  hormones.  The 
medullary  hormones,  thyroid,  and  pituitary  growth  and  thyrotropic  hor- 
mones have  a  number  of  interrelated  and  interdependent  actions.  We  have 
already  mentioned  that  growth  hormone  stimulates  adrenal  medullary 
hypertrophy  (Moon  ct  al.,  1950,  1951) .  Hypertrophy  of  the  medulla  with  a 
pronounced  increase  in  its  epinephrine  content  also  follows  chronic  poi- 
soning of  the  thyroids  of  male  and  female  rats  with  thiouracil  (Marine 
and  Bauman,  1945),  whereas  chronic  nicotine  poisoning  causes  a  marked 
medullary  hypertrophy  mainly  owing  to  an  increase  in  the  norepinephrine- 
containing  cells  (Eranko  et  al.,  1959).  The  latter  response  cannot  be 
elicited  in  mice  or  guinea  pigs,  although  the  adrenal  medulla  of  the  mouse 
has  both  epinephrine  and  norepinephrine-containing  cells  (Eranko  et  al., 
1959).  This  difference  clearly  demonstrates  the  kind  of  difference  one 
may  anticipate  between  species,  even  as  closely  related  as  arc  the  rat 
and  mouse  (Rattus  norvegicus  and  Mus  musculus).  The  ability  of  epi- 
nephrine to  mobilize  depot  fat  and  produce  a  rise  in  unesterified  fatty 
acids  in  intact  animals  is  thoroughly  established  (Hartman  andBrownell, 
1949),  but  its  ability  to  mobilize  depot  fat,  as  well  as  its  hyperglycemic 
action,  seems  to  depend  on  the  integrity  of  the  adrenal  cortex  and  the  carbo- 
hydrate-active corticoids  (Levy  and  Ramey,  1958;  DeBodo  and  Altzuler, 
1958).  These  results  have  led  Levy  and  Ramey  (1958)  to  suggest  that  the 
adrenal  steroids  and  epinephrine  may  act  in  concert  to  regulate  metabo- 
lism of  fat  cells.  On  the  other  hand,  the  ability  of  epinephrine  to  mobilize 
unesterified  fatty  acids  depends  on  optimal  thyroid  function,  but  appar- 
ently is  unrelated  to  adrenocortical  function  (Goodman  and  Knobil,  1959) . 
The  mobilization  of  fatty  acids  evidently  provides  a  readily  available  source 
of  metabolites  for  the  formation  of  glycogen  (Hartman  and  Brownell, 
1949)  or  for  direct  utilization  by  the  muscles  (Fredrickson  and  Gordon, 
1958). 


228  /•  /•  Christian 

It  is  well  known  that  epinephrine  produces  a  marked  rise  in  calorigenesis 
and  oxygen  consumption  in  intact  animals  (Hartman  and  Brownell,  1949; 
Gaddum  and  Holzbauer,  1957).  This  calorigenic  activity  of  epinephrine 
and  norepinephrine  is  important  in  adaptation  to  cold  (Hsieh  ct  al.,  1957a, 
b)  and  is  potentiated  strikingly  by  thyroxine  (Swanson,  1956,  1957)  and 
further  intensified  by  the  action  of  growth  hormone,  apparently  by  the 
effect  of  the  latter  in  increasing  thyroid  function  (Evans  ct  al.,  1958). 
Evidently  both  epinephrine  and  thyroxine  (and  an  intact  pituitary-thyroid 
system)  are  essential  for  survival  in  cold  exposure  and  for  adaptation  to 
cold,  as,  in  the  absence  of  thyroxine,  epinephrine  does  not  exert  its  calori- 
genic action  in  rats  (Swanson,  1956).  It  was  found  that  oxygen  consump- 
tion in  thyroidectomized  rats  increased  in  proportion  to  the  dose  of  epi- 
nephrine when  thyroxine  was  supplied  at  a  fixed  standard  dose  (Swanson, 
1957).  On  the  other  hand,  oxygen  consumption  varied  linearly  with  the 
log-dose  of  thyroxine  when  the  animals  were  kept  on  a  standard  dose  of 
epinephrine  (Swanson,  1956).  Epinephrine  apparently  is  essential  for  in- 
creased calorigenesis,  but  requires  thyroxine  for  its  activity.  Swanson 
(1957)  has  expressed  the  opinion  that  since  reactivity  to  epinephrine  is  di- 
rectly and  intimately  dependent  on  the  level  of  thyroxine,  the  main  role  of 
the  increased  secretion  of  thyroxine  in  acclimitization  to  cold  may  be  to 
potentiate  the  calorigenic  activity  of  epinephrine.  In  any  event,  both 
epinephrine  from  the  adrenal  medulla  and  an  intact  properly  functioning 
pituitary-thyroid  system  are  essential  for  increased  calorigenesis  and  adap- 
tation to  cold.  The  necessity  of  the  adrenal  medulla  and  an  intact  thyroid 
is  shown  by  the  fact  that  the  calorigenic  response  to  cold  is  abolished  either 
by  adrenal  demedullation  (Morin,  1946a,  b)  or  by  thyroidectomy  (Swan- 
son, 1957) . 

It  should  be  obvious  from  the  foregoing  discussion  that  the  adrenal 
medulla  and  sympathetic  nervous  system  and  their  hormones,  epinephrine 
and  norepinephrine,  are  vital  components  of  a  variety  of  adaptive  mecha- 
nisms and,  if  anything,  their  importance  has  tended  to  be  underestimated. 
It  appears  from  the  available  evidence  that  the  sympathico-adrenal  hor- 
mones may  play  a  key  role  in  the  physiologic  responses  to  sociopsychologic 
factors  associated  with  changes  in  population  density,  and  therefore  deserve 
more  investigation. 

B.  The  Thyroid  Gland 

1.  Introduction 

The  thyroid  gland  and  its  hormones,  primarily  thyroxine  and  to  a  lesser 
extent  triiodothyronine,  are  important  components  of  the  internal  mecha- 
nisms which  provide  the  organism  with  sufficient  physiologic  flexibility  to 


2.  Endocrines  and  Populations  229 

be  able  to  maintain  a  constant  internal  environment  in  the  face  of  stimuli 
from  and  changes  in  the  external  environment.  The  thyroid  is  intimately- 
associated  with  a  number  of  adaptive  mechanisms  and  has  important 
interractions  with  the  adrenal  cortex,  as  well  as  with  the  adrenal  medulla 
and  its  hormones.  This  is  not  the  place  for  a  detailed  account  of  thyroid 
physiology,  but  a  brief  review  of  the  functions  and  actions  of  the  thyroid 
and  its  hormones  will  be  given,  largely  derived  from  standard  accounts 
and  reviews.  Emphasis  will  be  placed  on  the  role  played  in  physiologic 
adaptation  to  environmental  changes,  especially  in  response  to  adverse 
stimuli. 

It  has  long  been  known  that  thyroid  hormone  is  essential  for  normal 
growth  and  development,  as  well  as  for  the  normal  metabolism  of  most 
tissues.  Furthermore  it  has  a  vital  role  in  permitting  a  mammal  to  adapt 
to  changes  in  the  temperature  of  the  external  envirormient,  especially  in 
adaptation  to  cold,  by  acting  synergistically  with  the  calorigenic  action  of 
epinephrine,  as  we  have  seen  in  the  preceding  section,  as  well  as  by  playing 
an  important  physiological  role  of  its  own  (Swanson,  1957) .  The  evidence 
that  will  be  discussed  subsequently  shows  that  the  thyroid  also  is  involved 
intimately  in  the  physiological  responses  to  alarming  stimuli.  Therefore 
the  thyroid,  like  the  adrenal  gland,  assumes  particular  importance  in  a 
discussion  of  adaptive  mechanisms. 

2.  The  Thyroid  Hormones  and  Their  Actions 

The  thyroid  hormones,  tetraiodothyronine  (thyroxine)  and  Z-3,5,3'- 
triiodothyronine,  have  an  overall  action  of  increasing  heat  production  by 
increasing  the  oxidative  processes  of  many  tissues  and  therefore  their 
oxygen  consumption  (SoUman,  1957).  This  metabolic  effect  is  in  part 
brought  about  by  the  augmentation  or  facilitation  of  the  calorigenic  action 
of  epinephrine,  and  it  has  been  suggested  that  thyroxine  and  epinephrine 
influence  consecutive  rate-limiting  reactions  in  the  metabolic  cycle,  thyrox- 
ine acting  at  a  later  stage  than  epinephrine  (Swanson,  1956) .  The  meta- 
bolic effect  of  thyroxine,  however,  is  not  exerted  equally  on  all  tissues.  The 
rate  of  oxidation  by  brain  tissue,  for  example,  is  not  influenced  at  all  (Tata 
et  al.,  1957),  whereas  the  metabolism  of  the  liver  is  increased  more  than  of 
the  body  as  a  whole  (Barker  and  Schwartz,  1953).  However,  the  electro- 
encephalogram excitability  of  the  brain,  electrolyte  distribution,  and  circu- 
lation of  the  brain  are  profoundly  affected  by  thyroid  hormones  (Tata 
et  al.,  1957).  Increased  thyroid  hormone  first  affects  carbohydrate,  then 
fat,  and  finally  protein  metabolism  (Sollman,  1957) .  Conversely,  a  defi- 
ciency in  thyroid  hormone  reduces  the  oxidative  activity  of  tissues  in 
general.  It  has  been  suggested  that  all  the  actions  of  the  thyroid  hormones 


230  /.  /.   Christian 

on  metabolic  processes  may  reflect  a  primary  action  at  one  biochemical  site, 
possibly  on  cytochrome  c  (Rawson  et  al.,  1955).  Sollman  (1957)  has  listed 
the  following  additional  effects  of  thyroxine.  Increased  levels  of  thyroid 
hormones  usually  are  accompanied  by  an  increased  pulse  rate,  increased 
nervous  excitability,  weight  loss,  and  decreased  liver  glycogen.  Thyroxine 
also  sensitizes  the  tissues,  especially  the  blood  vessels,  the  actions  of 
sympathomimetic  compounds  such  as  epinephrine  (see  above)  as  well  as 
to  the  toxic  effects  of  poisons.  The  increased  sensitization  apparently  occurs 
at  the  receptor  mechanisms.  Thyroxine  also  effects  the  circulation,  but 
mainly  as  a  result  of  increased  heat  production.  Thyroxine  has  a  direct 
effect  on  the  heart  in  increasing  its  oxygen  consumption,  but  it  also  has  an 
indirect  effect  on  the  heart  and  the  rest  of  the  circulatory  system  in  the 
following  way.  The  delayed,  indirect,  effect  is  due  to  an  increased  metabolic 
demand  of  the  tissues  which  results  in  an  increase  in  carbon  dioxide  and 
decreases  in  oxygen  at  the  arteriolar  level.  These  effects  result  in  a  subse- 
quent decrease  in  peripheral  resistance,  increased  venous  return,  and,  via 
cardiac  reflexes  such  as  the  Bainbridge,  an  increased  cardiac  output  and 
increased  pulse  pressure.  The  increased  heat  resulting  from  increased  oxida- 
tion must  be  dissipated,  and  this  is  accomplished  by  dilatation  of  the  vessels 
and  opening  of  the  arteriovenous  anastomoses  in  the  skin  and  other  tissues 
with  an  accompanying  increase  in  the  amount  of  heat  loss  due  to  radiation. 
The  normal  calorigenic  action  of  the  thyroid  hormones  is  essential  for 
normal  growth,  maturation,  and  tissue  differentiation. 

Proper  functioning  of  the  thyroid  and  the  production  of  thyroid  hormones 
is  completely  dependent  on  an  adequate  dietary  intake  of  iodine.  Inorganic 
iodine  is  essential  for  the  formation  of  the  thyroid  hormone  by  the  thyroid 
gland,  as  the  thyroid  is  incapable  of  trapping  organic  iodine  (Salter,  1949; 
Halmi  et  al.,  1953;  Rawson  et  al.,  1955) .  The  way  in  which  trapped  inorganic 
iodide  and  tyrosine  are  converted  into  the  thyroid  hormone (s)  has  been 
critically  reviewed  by  Rawson  et  al.,  (1955).  Inorganic  iodide  is  trapped 
and  presumably  momentarily  oxidized  to  active  iodine  in  the  thyroid  epi- 
thelium. Pituitary  thyrotropic  hormone  promotes  the  trapping  of  iodide 
by  the  thyroid  gland,  although  the  gland  has  some  autonomy  in  this  ac- 
tivity (Vander  Laan  and  Greer,  1950;  Halmi  et  al.,  1953;  Vander  Laan  and 
Caplan,  1954;  Vander  Laan,  1955).  The  trapped  and  activated  iodine  is 
then  used  in  converting  tyrosine  to  diiodotyrosine,  probably  in  the  presence 
of  peroxidases  and  cytochrome  oxidases.  Two  molecules  of  diiodotyrosine 
are  condensed  to  form  a  single  molecule  of  thyroxine  (tetraiodothyronine), 
which  combines  with  thyroid  globulin  (thyroglobulin)  and  is  stored  as 
such  in  the  colloid  of  the  thyroid  follicles.  Other  iodinated  thyronines  are 
found  in  the  thyroid,  but  in  much  smaller  amounts  than  thyroxine.  These 
probably  result  from  partial  iodination  and  may  represent  other  pathways 


2.  Endocrines  and  Populations  231 

in  the  formation  of  thyroxine  than  the  one  given  above  (Rawson  et  al., 
1955).  However,  the  metabohc  pathway  given  is  the  usually  accepted 
scheme  for  the  synthesis  of  thyroxine.  Thyroxine  presumably  is  released 
from  thyroglobulin  by  the  action  of  proteolytic  enzymes  (SoUman,  1957). 
Thyrotropin  promotes  the  iodination  of  tyrosine  and  the  release  of  thyrox- 
ine from  thyroglobulin.  A  small  amount  of  thyroxine  loses  one  of  its 
iodine  atoms  to  become  /-.3,5,o'-triiodothyronine  in  the  thyroid,  probably 
more  in  the  peripheral  tissues,  and  small  amounts  of  this  compound  are 
found  in  the  thyroid  gland  and  in  circulation  (Gross,  1955).  /-3,5,3'- 
Triiodothyronine  has  a  more  pronounced  action  on  oxidative  processes 
than  thyroxine,  and  its  action  is  much  more  rapid,  but  less  prolonged 
(Sollman,  1957).  It  has  been  postulated  that  the  triiodothyronine  provides 
the  immediate  thyroid  response  and  is  the  substance  which  enters  the  cells 
and  exerts  the  ultimate  thyroid  action,  whereas  thyroxine  is  the  circulating 
form  of  the  thyroid  hormone  (Gross,  1955).  The  differential  distribution 
of  these  two  hormones  in  the  circulation  and  in  the  cells  of  various  tissues 
supports  this  hypothesis  (Gross,  1955).  Since  inorganic  iodide  is  essential 
for  the  normal  functioning  of  the  thyroid  gland,  the  rate  at  which  radio- 
active iodine  is  trapped  and  accumulated  by  the  gland  is  a  good  index  of 
thyroidal  activity  provided  there  is  not  an  increased  renal  excretion  of 
iodine.  The  rate  of  release  of  radioactive  iodine  from  the  thyroid  is  con- 
sidered a  more  reliable  and  reproducible  index  of  thyroid  activity,  as  each 
animal  can  serve  as  its  own  control  in  experimental  procedures  (Brown- 
Grant  et  al.,  1954a). 

3.  Factors  That  Regulate  the  Activity  of  the  Thyroid 

The  secretory  activity  and  hormone  synthesis  by  the  thyroid  is  largely 
under  the  control  of  the  protein  hormone  thyrotropin  (TSH)  secreted  by 
the  basophils  of  the  anterior  pituitary  (D'Angelo,  1955).  However,  the 
thyroid  is  capable  of  a  low  level  of  autonomous  function  without  stimulation 
by  the  pituitary  (Brown-Grant  et  al.,  1954a).  The  rate  of  release  of  TSH 
from  the  anterior  pituitary  is  apparently  related  to  the  level  of  circulating 
thyroid  hormone  (D'Angelo,  1955),  although  the  mediation  of  the  hypo- 
thalamus appears  to  be  required  (Harris  and  Woods,  1958;  D'Angelo  and 
Traum,  1958) ;  therefore  a  low  level  of  circulating  thyroid  hormone  stimu- 
lates an  increased  secretion  of  TSH  and  a  high  level  inhibits  its  secretion. 
This  is  the  classic  concept  of  an  endocrine  feedback  mechanism  which 
operates  to  regulate  the  release  of  hormone  from  the  target  gland  and  to 
maintain  a  more  or  less  constant  level  of  circulating  hormone  under  normal 
circumstances.  However,  there  is  recent  evidence  to  suggest  that  the  secre- 
tion of  TSH  may  not  be  a  direct  response  to  the  level  of  circulating  thyrox- 


232  /.  /.  Christian 

ine,  but  rather  to  one  or  more  of  its  peripheral  metabohc  actions  (Goldberg 
et  al.,  1957) .  The  secretion  of  thyrotropin  from  the  pituitary  is  undoubtedly 
under  the  control  of  the  hypothalamus  (D'Angelo  and  Traum,  1958; 
Harris  and  Woods,  1958,  Harris,  1959).  Like  the  secretion  of  adrenocorti- 
cotropin,  the  secretion  of  thyrotropin  appears  to  be  controlled  by  a  humoral 
factor  from  the  hypothalamus  (Brown-Grant  et  al.,  1957).  Section  of  the 
pituitary  stalk  leads  to  a  loss  of  the  inhibitory  response  of  the  thyroid  to 
restraint  or  pain,  and  regeneration  of  the  pituitary  portal  vessels  is  accom- 
panied by  a  return  of  this  response  (Harris,  1955a,  b;  Reiclilin,  1957a,  b). 
The  thyroid  remains  at  least  partially  responsive  to  exogenous  TSH  when 
the  pituitary  stalk  is  sectioned  (Reichlin,  1957a,  b;  Harris  and  Woods, 
1958).  There  is  also  evidence  that  the  hypothalamus  is  necessary  for  a 
decreased  blood  concentration  of  thyroid  hormone  to  effect  an  increased 
secretion  of  TSH  (Greer,  1951;  1952;  Bogdanove  and  Halmi,  1953;  Harris 
and  Woods,  1958;  D'Angelo  and  Traum,  1958).  Increased  activity  of  the 
thyroid  accompanies  electrical  stimulation  of  the  anterior  portion  of  the 
median  eminence,  but  not  other  parts  of  the  hypothalamus  (Harris  and 
Woods,  1958).  The  hypothalamic  factor  responsible  for  stimulating  the 
release  of  TSH  does  not  appear  to  be  contained  in  or  associated  with 
Pitressin  (Reichlin,  1957a;  D'Angelo  and  Traum,  1958),  but  little  else  is 
known  of  the  nature  of  this  factor.  In  addition  to  these  regulating  factors, 
the  release  of  TSH  is  also  affected  by  cortisone  and  ACTH  (Brown-Grant 
et  al.,  1954c) .  Cortisone  and  ACTH  inhibit  the  release  of  radioiodine  from 
the  thyroid  of  rabbits  and  rats,  probably  by  inhibiting  the  release  of  TSH, 
although  the  mechanism  by  which  this  is  accomplished  is  unknown  (Brown- 
Grant  et  al.,  1954b,  c;  Brown-Grant,  1955).  Cortisone  and  ACTH  have 
also  been  reported  to  inhibit  the  uptake  of  radioiodine  by  the  thyroid  of 
rats  (Money  et  al.,  1950;  Albert  et  al.,  1952;  Perry,  1951;  Verzar  and  Vido- 
vic,  1952)  and  of  humans  (Perry,  1951;  Kuhl  and  Ziff,  1952;  Albert  et  al., 
1952;  Berson  and  Yalow,  1952).  However,  reports  of  decreased  uptake  of 
radioiodine  must  be  interpreted  with  caution,  as  it  may  reflect  a  lowered 
concentration  of  circulating  iodide  as  a  result  of  increased  renal  clearance 
of  iodide  following  treatment  with  cortisone  or  ACTH  (Brown-Grant  et  al., 
1954c) .  Adrenalectomy  inhibits  the  release  of  iodine  and  therefore  of 
thyroid  hormone  from  the  thyroid  glands  of  rats  (Fllickiger  and  Verzar, 
1955).  Bastenie  and  Ermans  (1958)  have  shown  that  cortisone,  in  addition 
to  its  effect  on  the  secretion  of  TSH,  inhibits  the  stimulating  effect  of 
thyroxine  on  oxygen  consumption  and  phosphorus  turnover  but  fails  to 
inhibit  these  actions  of  triiodothyronine.  These  authors  concluded  that 
cortisone  inhibits  the  peripheral  degradation  of  thyroxine  to  triiodothyro- 
nine. Nevertheless,  there  is  little  doubt  that  cortisone  and  ACTH  do  inhibit 


2.  Endocrines  and  Populations  233 

thyroid  activity,  probably  by  inhibiting  the  release  of  TSH  from  the  pitui- 
tary. 

Epinephrine,  in  addition  to  its  synergism  with  thyroxine  with  respect  to 
calorigenesis,  appears  to  have  more  direct  effects  on  thyroid  activity,  al- 
though the  reports  are  conflicting.  Badrick  and  his  co-workers  (1954,  1955) 
reported  that  epinephrine  inhibited  the  uptake  of  radioiodine  by  the  thy- 
roids of  intact  and  hypophysectomized  rats,  and  similar  results  were  re- 
ported for  intact  rats  by  Money  et  al.  (1950).  Brown-Grant  et  al.  (1954b) 
found  that  epinephrine  decreased  the  release  of  radioiodine  by  the  thyroids 
of  intact  rabbits.  On  the  other  hand,  epinephrine  has  been  reported  to  in- 
crease the  release  of  radioiodine  by  the  thyroid  (Williams  et  al.,  1949)  and 
to  increase  the  content  of  radioiodine  in  the  thyroid  (Botkin  and  Tew, 
1952),  although  the  latter  could  result  from  an  inhibition  of  its  release. 
More  recently  Ackerman  and  Arons  (1958)  have  reported  that  epinephrine 
increases  the  release  of  radioiodine  from  the  thyroids  of  intact  and  hypo- 
physectomized dogs.  These  divergent  results  are  difficult  to  reconcile,  al- 
though the  fact  that  epinephrine  acted  in  hypophysectomized  as  well  as  in 
intact  rats  and  dogs  (Badrick  et  al,  1955;  Ackerman  and  Arons,  1958) 
suggests  that  the  effects  were  directly  on  the  thyroid  gland.  It  is  possible 
that  these  varying  results  may  reflect  the  effects  of  epinephrine  on  the 
thyroidal  vasculature  in  different  animals  and  different  circumstances,  as 
was  suggested  by  Badrick  et  al.  (1955) . 

From  the  foregoing  account  it  should  be  clear  that  the  regulation  of 
thyroid  function  is  complex  and  involves  a  number  of  factors.  However, 
there  seems  to  be  little  doubt  that  the  hypothalamus  has  a  relatively  basic 
role  in  regulating  the  secretion  of  thyrotropin  from  the  pituitary,  and  evi- 
dently is  capable  of  inhibiting  its  release  (Brown-Grant  et  al.,  1954b;  Reich- 
lin,  1957a;  Harris  and  Woods,  1958;  D'Angelo  and  Traum,  1958) .  D'Angelo 
and  Traum  (1958),  in  an  extensive  series  of  experiments  on  hypothalamic- 
hypophyseal-thyroidal  relationships  in  rats,  concluded  that  the  hypothala- 
mus functions  as  a  modulator,  modifying  the  production  and  release  of 
thyrotropin  from  the  adenohypophysis  not  only  under  conditions  of  in- 
creased demand,  but  also  in  meeting  day-to-day  needs.  Normal  blood  levels 
of  TSH  cannot  be  maintained  in  the  absence  of  stimulation  by  the  hypo- 
thalamus even  with  decreased  levels  of  thyroid  hormone.  However,  the 
adenohypophysis  keeps  its  capacity  to  produce  and  release  TSH,  but  regu- 
lated by  the  level  of  circulating  thyroid  hormone  or  by  peripheral  meta- 
bolic actions  thereof  (cf.  above).  The  action  of  thyroid  hormone  may  be 
directly  on  the  pituitary  (D'Angelo  and  Traum,  (1958).  The  injection  of 
minute  amounts  of  thyroxine  into  the  hypothalamus  is  followed  by  a  rela- 
tively long  latent  period  before  the  release  of  TSH  is  inhibited,  whereas 


234  /.  J.  Christian 

injection  into  the  anterior  pituitary  is  followed  by  a  more  immediate  re- 
sponse (Yamada  and  Greer,  1959;  Yamada,  1959). 

The  thyroid  was  blocked  in  these  studies  (D'Angelo  and  Traum,  1958)  by 
hypothalamic  lesions  or  by  treatment  with  propylthiouracil.  Thyroid  func- 
tion was  evaluated  by  the  rate  of  turnover  of  radioiodine,  bioassay  of  the 
pituitaries  and  plasma  for  their  content  of  TSH  and  thyroid  hormone,  and 
was  finally  correlated  with  the  results  of  detailed  histologic  studies  of  the 
glands  of  experimental  and  bioassay  animals.  The  results  appear  to  be 
definitive.  These  workers  were  further  able  to  demonstrate  the  independ- 
ence of  ovarian,  adrenocortical,  and  thyroid-regulating  parts  of  the  hypo- 
thalamus. How  the  adrenal  corticoids  and  adrenocorticotropin  fit  into  the 
regulation  of  thyroid  function  is  unknown  at  present. 

4.  Thyroid  Responses  to  External  Stimuli 

Probably  the  best-known  action  of  the  tl\yroid  is  its  ability  to  increase 
the  secretion  of  thyroid  hormone  in  response  to  cold  and  to  decrease  its  re- 
lease in  response  to  heat  (Ring,  1942;  Brown-Grant  et  at.,  1954a;  Stevens 
et  al.,  1955;  Hellman  and  Collins,  1957).  The  gland  also  becomes  hyper- 
plastic with  prolonged  exposure  to  cold  (Money,  1955).  Upon  exposure  to 
low  temperatures,  there  is  an  increased  release  of  thyroid  hormone  from 
the  thyroid  gland  (Brown-Grant  et  al.,  1954a;  Woods  and  Carlson,  1956), 
apparently  in  response  to  increased  secretion  of  TSH  by  the  pituitary 
(Stevens  et  al.,  1955) .  However,  Ring  (1942)  found  that  the  thyroid  per  se 
can  account  for  only  a  small  part  of  the  increase  in  heat  production  required 
to  meet  the  demands  of  mice  exposed  to  2-4°  C,  and  that  epinephrine  and 
thyroid  hormone  are  required  for  a  maximum  continued  metabolic  response 
to  cold.  He  therefore  concluded  that  the  principle  action  of  thyroid  in  re- 
sponse to  cold  exposure  was  to  sensitize  the  animal  to  the  calorigenic  action 
of  epinephrine.  Swanson  (1956,  1957)  essentially  confirmed  and  extended 
Ring's  findings  and  conclusions,  as  we  have  seen  above.  Thyroidectomy 
abolishes  the  response  to  cold  and  deprives  the  animal  of  the  ability  to 
survive  exposure  to  cold  (Swanson,  1957). 

Exposure  to  cold  increases  pituitary-adrenocortical  as  well  as  thyroidal 
activity,  contrary  to  the  more  usual  reciprocal  relationship  between  ACTH 
and  TSH  secretion  by  the  pituitary  described  below.  Increased  thyroidal 
activity  apparently  is  the  more  vital  of  these  two  adaptive  responses  to 
cold.  Thyroxine  permits  survival  of  hypothyroid  rats  subjected  to  cold, 
whereas  adrenocorticotropin  and  adrenocortical  extract  do  not  (Freedman 
and  Gordon,  1955).  Similarly,  minute  amounts  of  thyroxine  permitted 
100%  survival  of  rats  treated  with  thiouracil  upon  exposure  to  cold, 
whereas  adrenocortical  extracts  failed  to  increase  survival  (Ershoff,  1948). 


2.  Endocrines  and  Populations  235 

Heat,  on  the  other  hand,  diminishes  thyroid  activity  (Hellman  and  Collins, 
1957) .  Rats  and  mice  have  been  shown  to  have  reduced  thyroid  activity  in 
the  warm  summer  months  (Hurst  and  Turner,  1947) .  Puntriano  and  Meites 
(1951)  suggested  that  the  seasonal  changes  in  thyroid  activity  might  be 
due  to  changes  in  day  length  rather  than  to  seasonal  changes  in  tempera- 
ture, on  the  basis  of  their  findings  that  prolonged  exposure  to  light  inhibits 
thyroid  activity  in  mice  and  exposure  to  darkness  increases  thyroid  activity. 
However,  these  results  could  not  be  confirmed  in  rabbits  (Brown-Grant 
et  al.,  1954b) .  The  rate  of  release  of  radio  iodine  by  the  thyroids  of  rabbits 
was  unaffected  by  prolonged  exposure  either  to  darkness  or  to  light.  There- 
fore the  validity  of  the  hypothesis  that  day  length  affects  thyroid  activity 
is  dubious. 

In  contrast  to  the  thyroidal-adrenocortical  relationship  in  response  to 
exposure  to  cold,  there  is  abundant  evidence  indicating  that  the  activity  of 
the  thyroids  is  inhibited  in  response  to  alarming  stimuli  which  evoke  an 
increased  pituitary-adrenocortical  activity.  In  fact  it  has  come  to  be 
generally  accepted  that  there  is  a  reciprocal  relationship  between  ACTH 
and  TSH  secretion  in  response  to  "stress"  (Harris,  1955a).  Anoxia,  neph- 
rectomy, vitamin  deficiencies,  tourniquet  shock,  fasting,  injected  typhoid 
vaccine,  swimming  in  cold  water  (15°  C),  injections  of  formalin,  spinal 
cordotomy,  electroshock,  and  gastrointestinal  and  peripheral  trauma  all 
inhibit  the  activity  of  the  thyroid  glands  of  rats  (Williams  et  al,  1949; 
Paschkis  et  al,  1950;  Bogoroch  and  Timiras,  1951;  Hamolsky  et  al,  1951; 
Van  Middlesworth  and  Berry,  1951;  Badrick  et  al,  1954,  1955).  Similar 
results  were  obtained  with  rabbits,  using  the  release  of  radioiodine  as  the 
basis  of  measurement  (Brown-Grant  et  al,  1954b) .  Hemorrhage,  anesthesia, 
laparotomy,  intraperitoneal  injections  of  turpentine,  draining  subcutaneous 
abscesses,  or  emotional  stress  in  the  form  of  restraint,  subcutaneous  faradic 
stimulation,  or  sudden  changes  from  light  to  dark  or  vice  versa  were  all 
found  to  inhibit  thyroid  activity  in  rabbits  for  1-2  days.  After  this  time 
the  rabbits  evidently  became  used  to  the  procedures  and  were  no  longer 
alarmed  by  them,  as  thyroid  function  gradually  returned  to  normal  with 
continued  exposure  to  these  emotional  stimuli.  In  some  instances  of  these 
experiments  the  release  of  radioiodine  from  the  thyroid  was  totally  inhi- 
bited (Brown-Grant  et  al,  1954b).  Emotional  stimuli  also  were  followed 
by  a  time  lag  of  somewhat  less  than  3  hours  between  their  first  application 
and  the  first  thyroid  response.  These  experiments  demonstrate  that  emo- 
tional factors,  visual  stimuli,  and  the  central  nervous  system  can  affect 
thyroid  function,  presumably  via  the  hypothalamus,  just  as  they  can  in- 
crease the  secretion  of  ACTH  or  adrenal  medullary  hormones  (Brown- 
Grant  et  al,  1954b).  Kracht  (1954)  described  a  "true  thyrotoxicosis"  in 
wild  rabbits  stimulated  by  fear  in  response  to  being  chased  by  ferrets  and 


236  /•  /•   Christian 

stated  that  this  reaction  was  a  "model  of  a  thyrotrophic  alarm  reaction." 
His  evidence  was,  for  the  most  part,  indirect  and  his  conclusions  that  there 
was  increased  thyroid  activity  on  the  basis  of  increased  renal  excretion  of 
injected  radioiodine  is  untenable,  especially  for  animals  with  a  marked 
increase  in  adrenocortical  activity.  Finally,  Brown-Grant  et  al.    (1954b) 
found  that  the  release  of  radioiodine  by  the  thyroids  of  wild  rabbits  was 
inliibited  by  emotional  stress,  just  as  it  was  in  the  usual  domestic  rabbit. 
In  view  of  these  facts,  the  concept  of  a  "fright  thyrotoxicosis"  is  inaccepta- 
ble.  The  inhibition  of  thyroid  activity  by  emotional  stress  was  not  as 
consistent  in  adrenalectomized  as  it  was  in  intact  rabbits,  otherwise  their 
responses  were  similar.  There  is  little  doubt  that  the  inhibition  of  thyroid 
activity  in  these  experiments  was  due  to  a  decrease  in  the  secretion  of 
thyrotropin  (Brown-Grant  et  al.,  1954a,  b).  It  is  true  that  cortisone  and 
ACTH  inhibit  the  release  of  TSH  from  the  pituitarics  of  rats  (Brown- 
Grant,  1955)  and  rabbits  (Brown-Grant  ct  al.,  1954a,  b;  Harris  and  Woods, 
1958),  but  the  adrenal  corticoids  cannot  be  responsible  for  the  major  part, 
if  any,  of  the  inhibition  of  the  release  of  thyrotropin  in  the  above  experi- 
ments, as  adrenalectomy  did  not  affect  the  inhibition  of  the  thyroid,  and 
cortisone  was  without  effect  on  the  release  of  radioiodine  by  the  thyroids 
of  rabbits  injected  with  TSH.  Finally,  injected  TSH  abolishes  the  inhibition 
of  the  thyroid  produced  by  ACTH  and  cortisone  (Harris  and  Woods,  1958) . 
There  has  been  some  confusion  in  the  literature  about  the  effect  of 
alarming  stimuli  on  the  thyroid.  The  experiments  which  have  been  dis- 
cussed so  far  indicate  that  the  thyroid  is  depressed  under  these  circum- 
stances, but  it  is  also  well  known  that  emotional  factors  can  precipitate 
thyroid  crises  in  humans  (Selye,  1950).  These  apparently  contradictory 
experiences  may  depend  on  species  differences,  dose-time  relationships,  or 
both,  as  the  recent  work  of  Gerwing  et  al.  (1958)  has  indicated.  These 
investigators  have  shown  that  thyroid  activity  in  mice,  rats,  and  rabbits 
subjected  to  chronic  toxic  "stress"  (injection  of  small  doses  of  bacterial 
endotoxins  repeatedly  for  24  days)   is  inhibited  at  first,  but  returns  to 
normal  and  eventually  exceeds  the  original  normal  level  as  the  stimulation 
continues  (Gerwing,  1958;  Gerwing  et  al,  1958) .  On  the  other  hand,  guinea 
pigs  and  rhesus  monkeys  exhibit  increased  thyroid  activity  from  the  be- 
ginning. Injected  thyrotropin  stimulates  the  thyroids  of  mice,  rats,  and 
rabbits  with  inhibited  thyroids  following  injection  of  the  toxin,  indicating 
that  the  depressed  thyroid  function  was  due  to  a  diminished  secretion  of 
TSH.  It  may  be  coincidental,  but  it  is  nevertheless  interesting  that  these 
differences  in  thyroid  function  between  species  in  response  to  bacterial  toxin 
coincide  with  the  differences  in  the  secretory  patterns  of  the  major  corti- 
coids in  these  same  species  (cf.  above) :  rats,  mice,  and  rabbits  secrete  pri- 
marily corticosterone,  and  the  other  two  species,  hydrocortisone.  These 


2.  Endocrines  and  Populations  237 

investigators  (Gerwing,  1958;  Gerwing  et  al.,  1958)  conclude  that  the 
hypothesis  of  a  reciprocal  relationship  between  the  secretion  of  TSH  and 
ACTH  by  the  anterior  pituitary  is  compatible  with  the  evidence  for  rats, 
mice,  and  rabbits,  but  not  for  guinea  pigs  and  monkeys.  They  suggest  that 
in  the  latter  two  the  inhibitory  effect  of  increased  corticosteroids  does  not 
occur.  However,  we  have  seen  that  the  corticosteroids  probably  have  no 
effect  in  the  inhibition  of  TSH  release  and  thyroid  function  in  rats  and 
rabbits  in  response  to  alarming  stimuli;  so  that  some  other  explanation 
must  be  sought.  Man  is  probably  similar  to  guinea  pigs  and  rhesus  monkeys 
in  the  above  responses  of  the  thyroid  (Gerwing  et  al.,  1958) .  These  studies 
emphasize  the  importance  of  careful  comparative  work  as  well  as  the  im- 
portance of  dose-time  relationships  in  physiologic  functions  in  different 
species  of  mammal. 

Starvation  causes  a  profound  depression  of  thyroid  activity,  probably 
by  suppressing  the  secretion  of  TSH,  as  it  has  been  shown  that  acute  starva- 
tion markedly  depresses  the  blood  level  of  TSH  in  mice  and  rats  (Monej'-, 
1955).  However,  if  starved  animals  are  subjected  to  cold,  the  degree  of 
thyroid  depression  is  inversely  related  to  the  degree  of  the  reduction  in 
environmental  temperature  (Reichlin,  1957a).  The  thyroidal  iodine  release 
rates  at  the  lower  temperatures,  even  though  reduced,  are  appreciably 
higher  than  they  are  in  starved  animals  at  higher  temperatures.  The  mainte- 
nance of  body  temperature  appears  to  take  precedence  over  the  conserva- 
tion of  nutritive  reserves  and  tissues  in  the  regulation  of  thyroid  activity 
activity  (Reichlin,  1957a). 

In  contrast  to  some  of  the  above  relationships,  increased  thyroidal  ac- 
tivity increases  adrenocortical  activity  in  white  rats  (Wallach  and  Reineke, 
1949).  Administration  of  thyroxine  decreases  adrenal  ascorbic  acid  to 
minimal  levels  after  2-4  days  and  is  followed,  upon  continued  treatment 
with  adequate  doses  of  thyroxine,  by  a  progressive  increase  in  adrenal  size 
and  ascorbic  acid  content  which  reaches  maximum  in  4  weeks.  The  in- 
crease in  adrenal  weight  in  these  circumstances  is  roughly  proportional  to 
the  dose  of  administered  thyroxine.  There  is  a  narrow  dose  range  of  thyrox- 
ine in  which  there  is  no  effect  on  the  adrenals.  Perhaps  this  dose  range  repre- 
sents the  normal  physiologic  daily  secretion  rate.  It  may  be  that 'the 
adrenal  hypertrophy  observed  in  rats  exposed  to  cold  is  in  part  a  result  of 
the  increased  thyroid  activity.  On  the  other  hand,  adrenocortical  function 
does  not  seem  to  depend  on  the  presence  of  a  normally  functional  thyroid, 
as  thyroidectomy  does  not  cause  adrenal  atrophy  in  rats  although  the  zona 
fasciculata  is  reduced  in  width  (Hess,  1953),  nor  does  thyroidectomy  de- 
crease the  ability  of  the  adrenals  to  respond  to  stress  (Hess  and  Finerty, 
1952) .  The  adrenals  of  rats  treated  with  antithyroidal  compounds  (thioura- 
cil)  also  (were)  responsive  to  injected  adrenocorticotropin  (Freedman  and 


238  /.  J.  Christian 

Gordon,  1955),  although  others  report  adrenal  atrophy  following  blockade 
of  the  thyroid  with  antithyroid  compounds  (Seifter  et  al.,  1949).  Finally, 
thyroxine  augments  the  adrenal  hypertrophy  produced  by  growth  hor- 
mone, although  it  does  not  augment  the  adrenal  hypertrophy  produced 
by  ACTH  (Bois  and  Selye,  1957). 

There  are  many  other  factors  which  affect  thyroid  function.  A  high 
dietary  salt  intake  can  increase  thyroid  activity,  even  with  a  reduced  iodine 
intake,  in  a  rather  circuitous  way  (Isler  et  al.,  1958).  The  mechanism  ap- 
pears to  be  that  with  increased  NaCl  intake,  and  consequently  an  increase 
in  the  excretion  of  NaCl,  there  is  apparently  an  increase  in  the  excretion  of 
iodine  with  the  sodium  chloride.  The  loss  of  iodine  produces  a  low  serum 
concentration  of  iodine  and  secondarily  a  low  output  of  thyroxine.  The  low 
thyroxine  in  turn  stimulates  increased  secretion  of  TSH  from  the  pituitary 
and  a  resulting  stimulation  of  the  thyroid  gland. 

5.  Thyroidal-Gonadal  Ixterrelatioxships 

In  addition  to  its  more  or  less  reciprocal  relationship  with  the  adrenal 
cortex,  the  thyroid,  and  therefore  thyroidal  adaptive  responses,  has  im- 
portant effects  on  reproduction  and  the  reproductive  organs.  These  effects 
are  of  particular  interest  here,  because  they  may  occur  in  response  to 
alarming  stimuli  and  must  be  evaluated  as  possible  factors  producing  the 
changes  in  reproductive  function  seen  in  mammals,  especially  in  relation 
to  the  density  of  the  population.  A  thyroidal-gonadal  relationship  is  indi- 
cated by  the  tendency  for  goiters  to  occur  at  puberty,  during  pregnane}^, 
or  during  estrus;  also  the  serum  protein-bound  iodine  is  elevated  during 
pregnancy,  and  stilbesterol  increases  the  release  of  iodine  by  the  thyroids 
of  rats  and  mice  (Sollman,  1957) .  There  is  considerable  evidence  indicating 
that  hypothyroidism  is  associated  with  abnormal  ovarian  function  in 
humans  (Rawson  et  al.,  1955).  Cold  exposure  causes  a  marked  increase  in 
the  length  of  the  estrous  cycle  in  rats  which  is  prevented  by  administering 
thyroxine  (Dempsey  and  Uotila,  1940;  Denison  and  Zarrow,  1955) .  Thyrox- 
ine also  augments  the  recover}^  of  function  of  the  testes  of  immature  rats 
from  the  atrophic  effects  of  starvation  (Horn,  1955) .  The  recovery  of  the 
testes  in  this  case  is  not  related  to  the  recovery  in  body  weight,  and  the 
recovery  of  the  sex  accessories  following  treatment  with  thyroxine  was  only 
partial  and  required  a  greater  period  of  time.  jMaqsood  and  Reineke  (1950) 
found  that  high  temperatures  (30°  C.)  decreased  the  weights  of  the  testes 
and  seminal  vesicles  of  mice,  but  the  administration  of  thyroprotein  in- 
creased the  weights  of  these  organs  at  that  temperature.  Alild  hj^perthy- 
roidism  stimulated,  and  hypothyroidism  inhibited,  sexual  development  in 
growing  male  mice  (Maqsood  and  Reineke,  1950).  Higher  doses  of  thyroid 


2.  Endocrines  and  Populations  239 

hormone  decreased  the  weights  of  the  reproductive  organs.  However,  thy- 
roidectomy was  followed  by  an  increase  in  the  relative  weights  of  the  testes 
and  seminal  vesicles  of  rats  (Hess,  1953).  Hess  (1953)  concluded  that 
thyroidectomy  appears  to  sensitize  the  gonads  and  secondary  sex  organs 
to  the  actions  of  gonadtropins.  Hyperthyroidism  is  associated  with  ab- 
normal reproductive  function  in  female  rats  (Denison  and  Zarrow,  1955). 
Prolonged  exposure  to  2°  C.  resulted  in  a  marked  increase  in  the  length  of 
the  estrous  cycle:  proestrus  and  estrus  were  markedly  prolonged,  while 
metestrus  and  diestrus  were  shortened.  Treatment  with  50  fxg.  of  thyroxine 
corrected  these  changes.  On  the  other  hand,  brief  (3  days)  treatment  of 
female  rats  with  thyroid  hormone  just  prior  to  mating  apparently  resulted 
in  an  increase  in  subsecjuent  litter  size,  but  the  same  amount  of  thyroid 
hormone  given  during  hot  weather  or  an  increased  amount  of  thyroid 
hormone  adversely  affected  reproduction  (Kraatz,  1939) .  These  results 
seem  to  indicate  that  there  is  an  optimal  level  of  thyroid  function  for  proper 
functioning  of  the  reproductive  system,  and  that  any  appreciable  deviation 
from  this  level  in  either  direction  leads  to  diminished  reproductive  function. 

The  role  of  the  thyroid  in  reproduction  has  been  investigated  by  using 
radiothyroidectomized  mice  (Bruce  and  Sloviter,  1957).  Radiothyroidec- 
tomy  apparently  had  no  effect  on  male  fertility,  female  fertility,  or  litter 
frequency,  but  estrus  and  gestation  were  prolonged.  Litter  size  possibly 
was  reduced  owing  to  increased  resorption  of  embryos,  but  the  matter 
requires  further  investigation.  In  contrast  to  these  results,  Chu  (1944) 
found  that  surgical  thyroidectomy  in  rabbits  during  early  pregnancy  re- 
sulted in  total  loss  of  fetuses  due  to  resorption  or  abortion.  The  same  opera- 
tion in  later  pregnancy  resulted  in  the  young  being  stillborn.  Induction  of 
pregnancy  in  thyroidectomized  rabbits  was  followed  by  resorption  or 
abortion  of  the  embryos  or  a  prolongation  of  gestation,  the  young  subse- 
quently being  born  dead.  Feeding  desiccated  thyroid  tended  to  prevent 
the  adverse  effects  of  thyroidectomy  (Chu,  1944) .  Hypothyroidism  induced 
by  thiouracil  in  rats  apparently  was  without  effect  on  fertility  in  males  and 
females  but  interferred  with  gestation  in  pregnant  females,  causing  resorp- 
tion in  100%  of  the  animals  (Jones  et  al.,  1946).  Thiouracil  treatment  of 
rats  for  shorter  periods  of  time,  less  than  100  days,  did  not  prevent  some 
of  the  rats  from  delivering  litters,  but  the  young  exhibited  abnormal 
growth  and  development.  It  is  quite  evident  that  pregnancy  is  adversely 
affected  by  severe  hypothyroidism  with  a  marked  increase  in  intrauterine 
mortality. 

In  general  reproductix'e  function  is  altered  by  marked  increases  or  de- 
creases in  thyroid  activity,  and  it  seems  that  the  depression  in  thyroid 
activity  in  response  to  alarming  stimuli  must  play  a  role,  along  with  in- 
creased adrenocortical  activity,  in  the  commonly  observed  suppression  of 


240  /.  /.  Christian 

reproductive  function.  However,  a  great  deal  more  investigation  is  neces- 
sary to  clarify  thyroidal-adrenal-gonadal  relationships. 

6.  Other  Thyroid  Relationships 

In  addition  to  its  relationship  to  the  adrenals  and  gonads  and  to  its 
general  metabolic  effects,  the  thyroid  hormones  play  specific  roles  in  the 
maintenance  of  other  organs,  and  these  may  offer  means  of  evaluating 
thyroid  function  in  intact  mammals  in  the  field,  providing  they  are  used 
in  conjunction  with  other  information.  For  example,  the  harderian  glands 
of  rats  and  mice  are  maintained  by  thyroid  hormones  and  as  yet  unknown 
pituitary  factors  (Boas  and  Bates,  1954;  Hellman  and  Collins,  1957),  al- 
though the  thyroid  may  not  be  essential  for  the  maintenance  of  these 
glands  in  guinea  pigs  (Smelser,  1943).  Thyroid  hormone,  in  conjunction 
with  androgens,  is  required  for  the  maintenance  of  the  size  and  granulation 
of  the  cells  of  the  serous  tubules  of  the  submaxillary  glands  of  male  rats 
(Grad  and  Leblond,  1949) .  The  thyroid  alone  restores  the  number  of  these 
cells  to  normal,  but  does  not  affect  the  size  or  granulation. 

Thyroid  function  in  general  declines  moderately  with  age  and  increased 
size  (Hurst  and  Turner,  1947),  but  there  are  no  marked  effects  of  this 
decline  on  growth  or  other  functions. 

It  should  be  clear  from  the  above  account  that  the  thyroid  and  its 
hormones  participate  actively  in  the  adaptive  responses  and  probably  play 
a  significant  role  in  many  aspects  of  these  responses.  Investigations  on  the 
reactions  of  the  thyroid  with  changes  in  the  size  of  mammalian  populations 
and  its  relationship  to  adrenal  and  reproductive  functions  are  needed. 

C.  Other  Endocrine  Adaptive  Factors 

1.  Pancreatic  Islets 

A  discussion  of  the  physiology  of  the  pancreas  with  respect  to  glucose 
metabolism  is  not  properly  the  function  of  this  chapter,  but  it  should  be 
mentioned  as  one  of  the  important  endocrine  organs  regulating  glucose 
utilization.  The  hormone  best  known  of  the  islets  of  Langerhans  is  insulin, 
which  is  derived  from  the  beta  cells.  More  recently  glucagon,  presumably 
from  the  alpha  cells,  has  been  described.  Insulin,  in  brief,  increases  the 
utilization  of  glucose  and  in  many  of  its  actions  is  antagonized  by  cortisone. 
Glucagon,  on  the  other  hand,  is  a  hyperglycemic  factor.  The  function  of 
these  hormones  wdth  respect  to  the  generalized  adaptive  responses  has  been 
little  studied,  but  they  certainly  must  play  an  important  part  in  the  general 
economy  of  the  mammal  under  adverse  circumstances.  Anything  affecting 


2.  Endocrines  and  Populations  241 

the  mobilization  and  utilization  of  glucose  is  inevitably  going  to  have  an 
important  bearing  on  the  adaptive  responses  when  a  more  or  less  final 
picture  is  available  of  the  entire  scheme  of  responses.  The  frequency  of 
diabetes  in,  and  its  effect  on,  natural  populations  is  unknown,  although 
recent  work  indicates  that  under  certain  circumstances  it  is  far  from  being  a 
rare  disease. 

2.  Posterior  Pituitary 

The  posterior  pituitary,  or  neurohypophysis,  has  been  mentioned  re- 
peatedly in  the  discussions  on  the  regulation  of  the  secretion  of  the  hor- 
mones of  the  anterior  pituitary,  but  in  addition  the  posterior  pituitary  has 
important  functions  of  its  own  in  physiologic  adaptation.  A  lengthy  discus- 
sion will  not  be  indulged  here,  as  these  functions  more  properly  come  under 
the  purview  of  discussions  elsewhere  or  are  insufficiently  well  known  to 
warrant  elaboration  in  detail. 

a.  Antidiuretic  hormone.  The  posterior  pituitary  secretes  an  antidiuretic 
factor  (ADH)  which  is  important  in  regulating  the  reabsorption  of  water 
from  the  renal  tubules.  Section  of  the  pituitary  stalk  with  a  resultant 
denervation  of  the  posterior  pituitary  is  followed  by  diabetes  insipidus  as  a 
result  of  the  lack  of  antidiuretic  hormone.  ADH  is  apparently  manufac- 
tured in  the  hypothalamus  and  traverses  the  fibers  of  the  supraopticohy- 
pophyseal  tract  and  is  released  into  the  systemic  circulation  in  the  posterior 
pituitary.  A  vasopressor  activity  is  associated  with  the  antidiuretic  factor 
of  the  pituitary,  but  the  neurohypophyseal  hormone  is  much  less  effective 
in  this  activity  than  in  its  antidiuretic  action.  There  is  evidence  that  the 
secretion  of  ADH  is  increased  in  response  to  alarming  stimuli  as  well  as  to 
dehydration,  but  there  is  also  evidence  that  its  secretion  is  independent  of 
the  release  of  ACTH  from  the  anterior  pituitary  (cf .  above) .  For  a  more 
detailed  account  of  the  pituitary  antidiuretic  hormone,  the  reader  is  re- 
ferred to  the  recent  review  by  Thorn  ( 1958) . 

h.  Lipid  mobilizing  factor.  The  recent  work  of  Seifter  and  his  colleagues 
( 1959)  has  indicated  the  existence  of  a  posterior  pituitary  lipid-mobilizing 
factor  (LMF)  which  is  released  in  response  to  adrenal  corticoids.  The 
hormone  appears  to  be  a  peptide  capable  of  mobilizing  triglycerides  from 
the  mesenteric  fat  depots  following  injections  of  cortisone,  exposure  to  cold, 
or  subjection  to  other  stimuli  which  induce  an  increased  adrenocortical 
secretion.  The  lipid  mobilization  is  blocked  by  adrenalectomy  or  hy- 
pophysectomy.  The  pathway  for  lipid  mobilization  by  LMF  in  response  to 
alarming  stimuli  appears  to  be  anterior  pituitary,  adrenal  cortex,  and 
posterior  pituitary  (Seifter  et  al.,  1959). 


242  /•  J-   Christian 

3.  Other  Gland  Systems 

Many  other  glands,  organs,  and  organ  systems  participate  in  physio- 
logic adaptation,  either  directly  or  in  response  to  increased  adrenal  or 
other  hormones  or  diminution  thereof.  The  essential  integrative  role  of 
the  central  nervous  system  has  been  implicit  throughout  the  foregoing 
discussion.  The  gastrointestinal  tract  and  its  appendant  organs  such  as  the 
pancreas,  liver,  and  salivary  glands  also  actively  participate  in  adaptation 
(Ehrich  and  Seifter,  1948;  Selye,  1950;  Baker  and  Bridgman,  1954;  Baker 
and  Abrams,  1954).  The  responses  of  the  stomach  to  the  adrenocortical 
hormones  and  to  "stress"  have  been  reviewed  recently  by  Gray  and  Ramey 
( 1957)  and  will  not  be  discussed  further  here.  One  example  of  the  participa- 
tion of  the  digestive  organs  in  the  adaptive  responses  is  the  response  of  the 
serous  glands  to  increased  adrenocortical  secretion,  first,  with  a  loss  of 
zymogen  granules  followed  by  a  depletion  of  cytoplasm  nucleic  acids 
(Ehrich  and  Seifter,  1948) .  However,  as  most  of  these  reactions  are  second- 
ary to  the  increased  activity  of  the  pituitary  and  adrenal  cortex,  it  does 
not  seem  appropriate  here  to  dwell  on  them  in  detail  since  the  entire  organ- 
ism responds  to  one  degree  or  another  to  the  physiologic  alterations  subse- 
quent to  increased  activity  of  the  primary  adaptive  mechanisms. 

D.  General  Measurements  of  the  Endocrine  Adaptive  Responses 

1.  General 

The  actions  of  and  responses  to  specific  hormones  and  adaptive  mecha- 
nisms have  been  discussed,  but  often  there  are  responses  in  the  intact  ani- 
mal that  cannot  be  attributed  to  a  particular  hormone  or  system.  The 
splenic  hypertrophy  and  enlargement  of  the  nucleus  pulposus  which  is  seen 
in  mice  and  voles  are  examples  of  such  responses  (Clarke,  1953 ;  Chitty  et  al., 
1956;  Christian,  1959c).  The  lymphoid  tissue  of  the  spleen  is  involuted  by 
the  adrenal  glucocorticoids,  but  the  resultant  atrophy  is  more  than  offset 
by  increased  hematopoiesis  in  the  spleens  of  mice  and  voles  subjected  to 
social  stress  or  other  alarming  stimuli  (Dawson,  1956).  However,  it  is  not 
known  at  present  what  specifically  is  responsible  for  the  increased  hemato- 
poiesis. This  seems  to  be  the  proper  place  to  review  the  physiologic  adaptive 
reactions  and  to  do  it  in  terms  of  these  responses  which  are  found  in  the 
intact  animal  in  response  to  specific  stimuli.  Wherever  possible,  the  re- 
sponses will  be  related  to  the  specific  mechanisms  responsible  for  their 
occurrence. 

It  is  appropriate  first  to  discuss  the  stimuli  which  are  known  to  elicit 
endocrine  adaptive  responses.  These  stimuli  have  typical  dose-time-re- 
sponse relationships:  the  severer  or  the  longer  the  stimulation,  the  greater 


2.  Endocrines  and  Populations  243 

the  response  (Sayers  and  Sayers,  1949).  Nevertheless,  it  should  not  be 
assumed  that  all  adaptive  responses  are  qualitatively  similar.  Some  stimuli 
may  elicit  quite  similar  responses  in  kind  and  degree,  whereas  others  will 
be  manifested  in  qualitatively  and  quantitatively  different  manners.  This 
statement  should  be  apparent  from  the  preceding  accounts  of  the  mecha- 
nisms involved.  All  have  in  common  that  if  uncompensated  they  will  pro- 
duce widespread  physiologic  changes  which  come  under  the  heading  of 
shock  (Selye,  1950).  However,  whether  or  not  the  symptoms  commonly 
associated  with  shock  are  elicited  depends  on  the  severity  of  the  stimulus 
(Selye,  1950).  In  short,  almost  any  change  in  the  external  environment  of 
the  animal,  physical  damage  to  the  animal,  or  emotional  upset  is  po- 
tentially capable  of  producing  profound  deleterious  effects  on  the  animal, 
but  within  reasonable  limits  the  adaptive  mechanisms  provide  it  with  the 
flexibility  to  accommodate  most  changes  or  adverse  stimuli.  It  is  not  until 
the  latter  reach  rather  serious  proportions  and  evoke  marked  responses  that 
we  customarily  consider  the  animal  to  be  subject  to  "stress"  or  that  the 
stimuli  are  labeled  "alarming." 

These  stimuli  act  through  or  are  received  through  a  number  of  receptor 
pathways  in  the  host.  For  example,  physical  damage,  including  burns, 
surgery,  acute  physical  trauma  of  all  sorts,  will  evoke  adaptive  responses 
probably  by  direct  neural  pathways  as  well  as  via  unknown  chemical 
mediators,  resulting  from  tissue  damage  which  may  stimulate  the  release 
of  ACTH  or  other  adaptive  responses  directly.  Hemorrhage,  by  decreasing 
blood  volimie,  can  effect  the  release  of  aldosterone  directly.  Many  stimuli 
act  directly  through  the  central  nervous  system,  such  as  light,  noise,  fear, 
and  rage.  Heat,  cold,  and  similar  environmental  factors  act  through  recep- 
tors in  the  individual  and  produce  physiologic  responses  directly.  Regardless 
of  the  receptor  or  pathway,  all  these  primary  environmental  stimuli,  if 
severe  enough  or  chronic  enough,  have  the  ability  to  produce  the  symptoms 
of  shock  through  several  common  pathways.  In  so  doing  they  produce 
physiologic  changes,  either  directly  or  through  the  central  nervous  system, 
which  in  turn  activate  a  chain  of  responses  leading  to  defensive  reaction 
against  any  adverse  physiologic  shift  which  may  have  been  produced  bj^ 
the  initial  stimulus.  The  variety  of  stimuli  which  have  been  found  to  elicit 
adaptive  reactions  is  legion  (Selye,  1950).  In  fact,  one  could  conclude  that 
almost  every  experience  encountered  in  daily  life  by  any  mammal,  if  severe 
enough,  is  capable  of  producing  shock  and  evoking  marked  adaptive  re- 
sponses, but  for  the  purpose  of  the  present  discussion,  the  importance  of 
emotional  stimuli  in  evoking  these  reactions  will  be  emphasized.  These 
may  be  acute  stimuli,  and  all  of  us  are  personally  familiar  with  some  of  the 
reactions  that  they  are  capable  of  producing.  However,  it  should  be  noted 
that  emotional  stimuli  of  a  more  chronic  nature  can  produce  profound 


244  /.  /.  Christian 

long-term  physiologic  reactions  which  may  have  widespread  effects  on  the 
host  (Elmadjian  ct  at.,  1958;  Ratcliffe  and  Cronin,  1958;  Christian,  1959b; 
Mason,  1959) .  Close  confinement,  for  example,  can  result  in  atrophy  of  the 
adaptive  mechanisms  with  a  resultant  marked  susceptibility  to  any  subse- 
quent alarming  stimulus  (Christian  and  Ratcliffe,  1952).  Social  competi- 
tion, social  pressure,  or  chronic  anxiety  have  been  shown  to  produce  marked 
physiologic  responses  (Christian,  1959a;  Mason,  1959)  Emotional  factors, 
of  necessity,  have  their  begmning  in  the  central  nervous  system  as  a  result 
of  sensory  stimuli  received  from  the  external  environment  via  the  visual, 
aural,  and  olfactory  sensory  receptors.  Therefore  they  are  part  of  the  com- 
plex system  responsible  for  integrating  the  mammal  with  its  immediate 
environment. 

Whatever  the  particular  adverse  stimulus  may  be  and  regardless  of  the 
pathway  of  its  reception  by  the  animal,  whether  sensory,  traumatic,  or 
otherwise,  all  stimuli  have  the  ability  to  produce  shock  and  evoke  physio- 
logic adaptive  responses  in  the  mammal.  Shock  is  a  vaguely  defined  entity, 
and  there  is  no  general  agreement  with  regard  to  its  exact  nature  or  cause 
(see  Selye,  1950;  Scudder,  1952;  Bing,  1952;  Zweifach,  1952;  Engel,  1952; 
Agate,  1952;  Randall,  1952).  Moreover,  its  primary  effects  and  the  re- 
sponses to  it  are  often  difficult  to  separate.  Fundamentally  the  principal 
manifestations  of  shock  involve  changes  in  the  circulatory  system  leading 
to  inadequate  circulatory  function  and  eventually  circulatory  collapse.  If 
one  keeps  in  mind  that  the  fundamental  changes  seem  to  be  blood  sludging 
(Knisely  et  at.,  1947),  changes  in  selective  permeability  of  the  peripheral 
vascular  bed  (Zweifach,  1952),  capillary  and  arteriolar  atony  and  hypo- 
reaction  of  the  vascular  musculature  to  epinephrine  and  even  direct  stimu- 
lation with  a  resultant  visceral  vasodilatation  (Zweifach,  1952),  then  the 
secondary  effects  of  hemoconcentration,  hypotension,  fall  in  blood  volume, 
decreased  cardiac  output,  and  increased  hematocrit  are  more  easily  under- 
stood. In  general  shock  seems  to  involve  a  generalized  failure  of  circulatory 
integrity.  With  a  superimposed  relative  circulatory  stasis  and  hypoxia, 
these  alterations  became  more  profound  and  the  whole  process  becomes  a 
vicious  circle  (Zweifach,  1952) .  It  is  not  known  by  what  specific  means 
these  alterations  in  circulation  are  effected,  but  that  they  involve  both 
neural  and  humoral  components  can  hardly  be  questioned.  We  have  re- 
peatedly emphasized  that  the  basic  role  of  the  adaptive  reactions  is  to 
maintain  vascular  integrity  in  opposition  to  forces  tending  to  destroy  it  as 
well  as  to  prepare  the  organism  to  meet  emergency  situations.  Most 
alarming  stimuli  also  produce  an  immediate  discharge  of  the  medullary 
hormones  epinephrine  and  norepinephrine  with  mobilization  of  glucose 
reserves,  increased  heart  rate  and  strength  of  beat  increasing  cardiac  out- 
put, and  constriction  of  visceral  and  cutaneous  blood  vessels  with  a  re- 


2.  Endocrines  and  Populations  245 

suiting  shift  of  the  blood  to  the  musculature  and  central  nervous  system.  If 
at  the  same  time  the  output  of  ACTH  is  stimulated,  adrenal  glucocorticoids 
will  also  be  increased,  and  among  their  effects  is  an  increase  in  the  reactivity 
of  the  blood  vessels  to  the  effects  of  epinephrine  and  norepinephrine.  None 
of  the  reactions  is  completely  isolated,  but  the  balance  between  them  may 
shift  considerably  depending  on  the  nature  of  the  stimulus.  There  are  also 
temporal  relationships.  In  many  ways  the  immediate  and  short-lived  ac- 
tivity of  epinephrine  is  mimicked  more  chronically  by  the  glucocorticoids. 
The  response  of  the  adrenal  medulla  is  immediate,  whereas  the  cortex  re- 
sponds somewhat  more  slowly  and  can  be  sustained  for  a  great  deal  of 
time. 

It  is  worth  describing  some  of  the  measurable  changes  effected  in  a 
mammal  by  activation  of  the  adaptive  responses.  Many  of  these  have  been 
described  earlier  under  actions  of  the  various  hormones,  but  it  is  appropri- 
ate to  discuss  these  as  measurable  changes  in  such  a  way  that  they  might  be 
useful  in  detecting  and  interpreting  the  effects  of  physiologic  adaptation  to 
potentially  harmful  stimuli.  Many  of  the  actions  of  the  adrenocortical  and 
other  hormones  serve  specifically  to  restore  the  equilibrium  of  the  internal 
environment  after  an  alarming  stimulus.  These  actions  may  be  a  decided 
disadvantage  to  the  animal  when  they  are  prolonged.  For  example,  in- 
creased secretion  of  the  carbohydrate-active  corticoids  serves  to  maintain 
proper  fluid  and  electrolyte  balances  and  to  provide  readily  available 
glucose  reserves  in  an  emergency,  but  they  also  suppress  inflammation, 
granulation,  and  antibody  formation  and  thereby  reduce  host  resistance  to 
infection.  The  following  list  of  adaptive  responses  is  by  no  means  complete, 
those  effects  having  been  selected  which  might  prove  useful  or  of  basic  con- 
ceptual importance  to  the  investigator  wishing  to  study  the  effects  of 
physiologic  adaptation. 

2.  Measurements  Indicating  Increased  Adrenocortical  Function 

a.  Adrenal  weight.  This  is  a  presumptive  measurement  of  ACTH  activity 
and  response  to  an  acute  or  chronic  distress.  Weights  are  the  best  available 
index  of  adrenocortical  activity  for  many  studies.  For  many  long-term 
changes,  weights  are  much  more  useful  and  much  less  subject  to  pitfalls 
than  techniques  such  as  changes  in  adrenal  ascorbic  acid,  lipids,  cholesterol, 
the  production  of  plasma  corticosteroids,  or  circulating  eosinophils  or 
lymphocytes.  All  these  measurements  are  labile  and  reflect  rapid  changes 
in  adrenal  function:  they  indicate  the  status  of  the  animal  at  the  moment 
of  making  the  measurement,  thus  they  often  may  reflect  nothing  more 
than  the  process  of  trapping  or  handling  an  animal  and  may  completely 
obscure  longer-term  changes,   especially  with  excitable,  highl}^  reactive 


246  J.  J.  Christian 

wild  mammals.  The  problem  of  adrenal  hypertrophy  with  weight  changes 
and  the  relationship  of  these  changes  to  the  length  and  severity  of  the 
stimulus  has  been  discussed  at  length  by  Sayers  and  Sayers  (1949). 

Adrenal  weight  changes  largely  reflect  changes  in  the  zona  fasciculata, 
which  undergoes  hyperplasia  and  hypertrophy  following  stimulation  by 
ACTH  and  therefore  is  indicative  of  changes  in  the  secretion  of  glucocorti- 
coids. Under  different  circumstances,  already  discussed,  the  zona  glomeru- 
losa  may  hypertrophy,  but  changes  in  its  size  are  relatively  unimportant 
with  respect  to  changes  in  the  total  weight  of  the  gland  because  of  its  rela- 
tively minor  contribution  to  the  total  mass  of  the  adrenal.  The  same  state- 
ment is  generally  true  for  the  adrenal  medulla.  Fmally,  changes  in  adrenal 
weight,  when  used  as  an  index  of  cortical  activity,  should  be  evaluated  in 
the  light  of  histologic  studies,  as  the  amount  of  lipid  in  the  cells  of  the 
fasciculata  may  vary  sufficiently  with  various  functional  states  to  have  a 
marked  effect  on  adrenal  weight.  For  example,  stimulation  of  sufficient 
intensity  to  deplete  the  cortex  of  visible  lipids  may  result  in  a  decrease  in 
weight  whereas  activity  may  actually  be  greater  than  in  a  heavier  gland 
containing  a  large  amount  of  stored  lipid  (Christian,  1959b). 

The  adrenal  medulla  in  some  species  may  hypertrophy  and  contribute 
to  changes  in  adrenal  weight.  Although  the  contribution  of  the  medulla  to 
adrenal  weight  changes  is  usually  negligible,  it  would  be  appreciable  for  the 
Soricidae,  in  which  the  adrenal  is  composed  largely  of  medulla.  Changes  in 
the  medulla  and  cortex  of  the  long-tailed  shrews  (soricids)  have  not  been 
studied,  although  these  medullary-cortical  relationships  have  been  ob- 
served in  mature  individuals  of  Sorex  cinereus,  S.  dispar,  S.  fumeus,  S. 
palustris,  and  Microsorex  hoyi.  There  may  be  a  relationship  between  body 
size,  adrenocortical  mass,  and  metabolic  rate  in  these  minute  animals. 

Adrenal  weights  are  useful  for  field  studies,  as  they  may  be  obtained  on 
fresh  or  fixed  material.  From  a  practical  standpoint  it  is  almost  essential  to 
obtam  adrenal  weights  from  fixed  material  in  mammals  the  size  of  mice  or 
smaller,  as  it  is  very  difficult  to  clean  the  glands  properly  and  obtain 
reliable  weights  on  the  fresh  glands.  Rapid  water  loss  from  fresh  glands  of 
such  small  size  further  complicates  the  problem.  In  most  circumstances 
increased  adrenal  weight  is  an  acceptable  indicator  of  increased  adreno- 
cortical activity,  but  an  effort  should  be  made  to  obtain  the  adrenals  from 
suddenly  killed  specimens,  not  from  captive  animals  or  from  those  held  in  a 
live  trap  for  extended  periods,  if  one  wishes  to  assess  adrenocortical  func- 
tion in  the  animal  in  its  natural  state.  Animals  which  have  been  killed  sud- 
denly are  not  subject  to  changes  in  adrenal  weight  resulting  from  capture 
or  handling 

A  word  of  caution  is  appropriate  here  with  regard  to  sample  size  and 
adrenal  weight-body  weight  relationships.  Adrenal  weight  varies  consider- 


2.  Endocrines  and  Populations  247 

ably  from  individual  to  individual  in  the  same  species,  as  well  as  with  sex, 
size,  and  therefore  presumably  age,  in  most  mammals.  Consequently,  it  is 
necessary  to  obtain  large  enough  samples  of  each  sex  in  each  age  category 
(at  least  mature  and  immature)  to  have  reliable  criteria  for  evaluating 
adrenal  changes  in  a  population  and  with  time. 

In  most  studies  of  natural  populations  the  investigator  does  not  have 
the  privilege  of  selecting  animals  for  size,  weight,  and  sex  each  time  a 
sample  is  collected,  and  it  is  therefore  necessary  to  determine  adrenal 
weight-body  weight,  adrenal  weight-body  length  or  some  similar  relation- 
ship for  the  species  and  population  with  which  he  is  working  in  order  to 
establish  a  common  baseline  for  all  samples.  For  example,  it  has  been  found 
that  the  logarithm  of  adrenal  weight  in  milligrams  on  body  length  (exclu- 
sive of  the  tail)  gives  the  best  straight-line  relationships  for  all  sizes  and 
for  both  sexes  of  Norway  rats  (Christian  and  Davis,  1955).  In  other 
species,  for  example  Microtus  pennsylvanicus,  there  are  marked  differences 
in  body-adrenal  relationships  between  the  sexes.  The  oft-used  relationhsip 
of  milligrams  of  adrenal  weight  per  100  grams  of  body  weight  is  quite 
arbitrary  and  usually  overcorrects  for  lighter  and  undercorrects  for  heavier 
animals,  therefore  this  relationship  is  useful  for  only  a  very  narrow  range 
of  body  weights.  Too  often  adrenals  are  collected  as  an  afterthought  along 
with  other  data  for  which  the  investigation  was  originally  designed.  As  a 
result  the  data  on  adrenal  weights  usually  are  inadequate.  A  study  of 
changes  in  adrenal  weight  must  be  designed  specifically  to  obtain  this  and 
the  necessary  related  data.  Samples  should  be  consistent  with  respect  to 
time,  size,  and  sex  and  age  composition.  Many  potentially  useful  field 
studies  involving  an  immense  amount  of  effort  have  been  of  little  value 
because  of  poor  sampling,  usually  a  result  of  treating  the  collection  of  data 
on  adrenal  weights  as  a  secondary  consideration  in  the  investigation. 
Sampling  usually  does  not  alter  the  populations,  as  the  normal  rate  of  re- 
cruitment more  than  offsets  losses  due  to  sampling,  in  addition  to  which 
compensatory  changes  in  mortality  rate  will  usually  compensate  for  sam- 
pling losses,  unless  the  sampling  is  very  frequent  and  intense. 

b.  Routine  Idstologic  measurements.  It  is  very  useful  in  evaluating  adrenal 
activity  to  measure  the  widths  of  the  various  zones  and  to  coimt  the  number 
of  cells  in  cortical  cords  in  an  area  where  cords  are  in  straight  columns 
(Zwemer,  1936;  Zwemer  et  al.,  1938;  Christian,  1956).  A  variation  of  this 
is  the  somewhat  more  precise  technique  of  projecting  a  section  through  the 
center  of  the  gland  and  outlining  the  various  zones,  cutting  them  out  and 
weighing  them  or  else  measuring  their  areas  with  a  planimeter.  It  is  possible 
with  serially  sectioned  glands  to  repeat  such  a  procedure  every  so  many 
sections  and  in  this  way  obtain  precise  information  on  the  contributions 
made  by  the  individual  zones  to  the  total  weight  of  the  gland.  These  tech- 


248  J-  J'  Christian 

iiiques  necessitate  selection  of  appropriate  sections  and  therefore  require 
serial  sections,  especially  on  animals  with  adrenals  weighing  less  than  20  mg. 
For  glands  heavier  than  this  it  is  possible  to  select  a  portion  through  the 
middle  of  the  gland  grossly.  All  these  measurements  can  be  made  on  sec- 
tions stained  routinely  with  hematoxylin  and  eosin. 

c.  Adrenocortical  sudanophilia.  Fat  stains,  such  as  Sudan  IV,  are  useful 
in  evaluating  the  secretory  activity  of  the  adrenal  cortex.  The  normally 
active  cortex  ("resting  cortex")  usually  exliibits  marked  sudanophilia  of 
the  fasciculata  and  glomerulosa  with  the  fat  in  vacuoles  of  moderate  size. 
The  sudanophilia  becomes  markedly  reduced  in  acutely  stimulated  glands, 
and  upon  continued  stimulation  the  sudanophilia  may  return  to  some 
degree,  but  the  size  of  the  vacuoles  is  reduced.  Upon  cessation,  sudanophilia 
becomes  intense  and  the  vacuoles  large.  Sudanophilia  may  be  completely 
absent  from  some  species,  such  as  the  hamster,  under  all  conditions.  These 
details  are  discussed  in  greater  detail  elsewhere  (Dempsey,  1948;  Sayers 
and  Sayers,  1949;  Greep  and  Deane,  1949a,  b;  Symington  ct  al.,  1958). 

d.  Adrenal  ascorbic  acid  declines  on  stimulation  with  ACTH,  and  this  has 
been  used  as  a  means  of  assaying  ACTH  in  blood  and  other  fluids  (Sayers 
and  Sayers,  1949;  Greep  and  Deane,  1949b).  In  general  adrenal  ascorbic 
acid  is  a  useful  histochemical  means  of  assaying  adrenal  stimulation,  al- 
though it  may  not  reflect  cortical  secretory  activity  in  the  intact  animal 
(Slusher,  1958). 

e.  Adrenal  cholesterol  also  declines  on  stimulation  and  can  be  used  to 
assess  cortical  activity  (Dempsey,  1948;  Sayers  and  Sayers,  1949;  Greep 
and  Deane,  1949b) .  IVIeasurement  of  adrenal  steroids  in  the  adrenal  venous 
affluent,  plasma,  or  their  urinary  metabolites  can  be  used  to  assess  cortical 
activity  directly.  These  are  subject  to  daily  variation  and  also  respond 
rapidly  to  stimulation.  In  addition,  they  are  involved  techniques  to  carry 
out  properly.  There  is  also  some  question  concerning  the  biologic  signifi- 
cance of  the  particular  steroids  measured.  These  measurements  are  there- 
fore of  limited  use  for  investigations  outside  the  laboratory  and  are  subject 
to  numerous  pitfalls  in  dealing  with  species  which  cannot  be  handled  in  the 
laboratory  without  stimulating  adrenal  activity.  Measurements  of  urinary 
corticosteroids  when  practical  and  appropriate  would  probably  be  more 
useful  than  blood  levels  for  studies  relating  to  mammalian  populations. 
Measurement  of  the  products  of  corticosteroid  metabolism  in  the  urme  is 
somewhat  intermediate  between  weight  and  direct  functional  measure- 
ments, such  as  plasma  steroids  as  a  measure  of  chronic  cortical  stimulation. 
Urinary  steroids  may  be  collected   daily  ad  infinitum  under  laboratory 
conditions  and  are  unquestionably  ^'ery  valuable.  However,  steroid  deter- 
minations are  complex  and  difhcult  to  interpret,  and  in  the  last  analysis 
represent  only  those  steroids  which  escape  as  such  via  the  urine  and  consist 


2.  Endocrines  and  Populations  249 

largely  of  metabolites  of  steroids.  Furthermore,  a  large  proportion  of  the 
steroids  are  excreted  in  the  feces  in  some  species  (Barry  et  al.,  1952;  Brad- 
low  et  al.,  1954) .  Finally,  techniques  for  the  measurement  of  urinary  steroids 
have  not  been  developed  for  use  in  the  field.  It  should  be  apparent  from 
the  foregoing  discussion  that  assessing  adrenocortical  activity  by  physio- 
logic or  biochemical  means  in  living  wild  animals  or  animals  which  have 
been  live-trapped  is  fraught  with  difficulty,  and  under  most  circumstances 
measurements  as  such  of  the  blood  steroids  will  probably  reflect  the  im- 
mediate situation  of  the  animal.  In  general,  weight  and  histologic  criteria 
from  animals  killed  suddenly  seem  to  offer  the  most  dependable  informa- 
tion at  the  present  time  for  studying  population  phenomena,  although  the 
other  procedure  should  be  explored  further.  Even  though  there  is  always  a 
question  of  the  presumptive  relationship  between  hypertrophy,  morpho- 
logic change,  and  function,  in  most  circumstances  it  is  generally  accepted 
that  cortical  hypertrophy  reflects  functional  change.  The  probable  depar- 
tures from  this  general  statement  have  been  discussed  above. 

/.  Thymicolymphatic  system.  In  the  foregoing  account  it  has  been  stated 
that  the  carbohydrate-active  corticoids  produce  involution  of  the  thymico- 
lymphatic system,  mainly  by  effecting  involution  of  the  lymphoid  elements 
proper.  Therefore  the  weights  of  the  thymus,  lymph  nodes,  and  spleen 
provide  useful  indicators  of  increased  adrenocortical  activity,  especially  if 
appropriate  histologic  checks  are  used. 

g.  Thymus  weight.  Cortisone,  hydrocortisone,  and  endogenous  adrenal 
corticoids  cause  thymic  involution  (Dougherty,  1952a,  b;  Weaver,  1955). 
Increased  phagocytosis,  edema  of  the  connective  tissue  stroma,  and  hyper- 
plasia of  the  reticulum  are  associated  with  the  destruction  of  the  lymphoid 
tissue  proper  (Gordon,  1955).  If  the  reaction  is  severe  enough,  only  the 
stroma  and  a  modified  reticulum  may  remain,  with  no  distinction  between 
the  cortex  and  medulla  of  thymus  and  lymph  nodes  in  degree  of  involution 
(Weaver,  1955) .  The  weight  of  the  thymus  therefore  will  be  greatly  de- 
decreased.  Within  a  few  hours  following  stimulation  there  is  a  marked 
edema,  and  during  this  period  of  edema  there  is  a  marked  reduction  in  the 
number  of  lymphocytes;  those  remaining  exhibit  degenerative  changes 
(Dougherty,  1952b).  The  effect  is  greatest  on  the  small  and  medium-sized 
lymphocytes  and  thymocytes.  These  exhibit  pycnosis,  karyolysis,  frag- 
mentation, and  other  degenerative  changes.  The  fragments  arc  phagocy- 
tized  by  macrophages  in  reaction  centers  of  the  lymph  nodes  and  are 
carried  off  in  the  lymphatics.  Mitosis  ceases  in  the  lymphoid  organs  with 
treatment  with  cortisone,  hydrocortisone,  or  with  chronic  stimulation  of 
the  adrenal  cortices  by  endogenous  or  exogenous  ACTH  (Weaver,  1955; 
Gordon,  1955) .  These  changes  are  common  to  the  lymphoid  follicles  where- 
ever   they    occur — spleen,    nodes,    gastrointestinal    tract — but    are    most 


250  /.  /.   Christian 

marked  in  the  thymus.  The  greater  sensitivity  of  thymocytes  over  the 
l3'mphocytes  of  the  lymph  nodes  and  spleen  may  be  due  to  their  greater 
rate  of  proliferation.  Hydrocortisone  is  more  effective  in  involuting  lymph- 
oid tissue  than  cortisone  or  corticosterone  and  tends  to  produce  degenera- 
tive changes  among  immature  lymphocytes  of  the  lymph  nodes  to  a  much 
greater  degree  than  cortisone  (Dougherty,  1953;  Santisteban  and  Dough- 
erty, 1954) .  On  the  other  hand,  pituitary  growth  hormone  and  deoxycorti- 
costerone appear  to  promote  the  growth  of  lymphoid  tissue.  The  apparent 
increase  in  lymphoid  tissue  in  gonadectomized  or  adrcnalectomized  animals 
(or  in  those  animals  with  adrenocortical  atrophy  from  inactivity  or  over- 
feeding) (Christian  and  Ratcliffe,  1952)  apparently  results  from  protection 
against  involuting  agents  rather  than  a  true  hyperplasia  (Dougherty, 
1953;  Santisteban  and  Dougherty,  1954) . 

The  thymus  is  also  involuted  by  androgens,  estrogens,  and  to  a  variable 
degree  by  thyroidectomy  (Weaver,  1955) ;  androgens  also  potentiate  the 
ability  of  the  carbohydrate-active  corticoids  to  effect  thymic  involution 
(Selye,  1955;  Dorfman  and  Shipley,  1956) .  The  cortex  and  medulla  remain 
as  distinct  zones  after  this  type  of  in\'olution,  although  they  are  less  clearly 
defined  than  in  normal  (Weaver,  1955).  All  elements  of  the  thymus  share 
equally  in  the  involutional  process.  Thymocytes  are  reduced,  but  no  acute 
destructive  changes  are  noted.  Therefore  involution  of  the  thymus  by  sex 
hormones  is  a  nonspecific  action  (Weaver,  1955) .  The  so-called  involution 
due  to  aging  of  the  thymus  is  prevented  by  gonadectomy  in  either  sex 
(Selye,  1947) ;  it  is  therefore  logical  to  assume  that  the  normal  involution 
of  this  gland,  seen  especially  at  puberty  (Christian,  1956) ,  is  due  to  the  sex 
steroids. 

It  should  be  clear  that  thymic  mvolution,  and  therefore  thymic  weight, 
can  be  a  useful  index  of  adrenocortical  activity  providing  adequate  con- 
sideration is  taken  of  the  action  of  the  sex  steroids. 

h.  Lymph  node  weight  is  a  useful  index  of  adrenocortical  activity  as  their 
lymphoid  tissue  is  involuted  by  adrenal  corticoids  as  discussed  above,  but 
to  a  less  marked  degree  than  for  the  tlwmus  (Weaver,  1955).  The  iliac 
lymph  nodes  are  involuted  by  prolonged  administration  of  ACTH,  acute 
administration  of  cortisone  (Weaver,  1955),  or  a  variety  of  stimuli  evoking 
increased  adrenocortical  activity,  but  the  sex  steroids  or  thyroidectomy  are 
without  effect  on  the  lymph  nodes  (Weaver,  1955)  or  maj^  even  cause  a 
hypertrophy  of  the  nodes  (Money  et  al.,  1950) .  A  weight  loss  of  the  lymph 
nodes  should  therefore  more  specifically  reflect  increased  adrenocortical 
activity  than  the  thymus.  However,  tlwmic  weight,  properly  controlled,  is 
more  frequently  used  because  of  its  much  greater  sensitivity  to  the  carbo- 
hydrate-active corticoids.  Selye  (1950)  has  indicated  that  the  lymph  nodes 


2.  Endocrines  and  Populations  251 

even  may  hypertrophy  in  infectious  disease  or  other  circumstances  which 
demand  increased  phagocytosis  of  particulate  matter. 

i.  Splenic  weight.  The  lymph  follicles  of  the  spleen  also  are  involuted  by 
corticoids.  A  decrease  in  splenic  weight  is  sometimes  used  to  indicate  in- 
creased adrenocortical  secretion.  However,  contraction  of  the  splenic  cap- 
sule further  decreases  splenic  weight.  This  may  be  a  valid  procedure  in  the 
laboratory  with  injected  hormones,  particularly  in  hypophysectomized  or 
adrenalectomized  animals,  but  often  may  not  be  a  valid  indication  of  in- 
creased adrenocortical  activity,  especially  in  intact  small  mammals.  Voles 
(Microtus  agrestis)  and  house  mice,  and  possibly  other  species,  exhibit  a 
marked  splenic  hypertrophy  when  exposed  to  stimuli  which  cause  in- 
creased pituitary-adrcnocortical  activity  (Clarke,  1953;  Chitty,  1957; 
Christian,  1959c).  Ln-olution  of  the  lymphoid  follicles  occurs  in  these 
circumstances  as  might  be  expected,  but  the  decrease  in  weight  from  this 
cause  is  overridden  by  the  hypertrophy  resulting  from  congestion  and 
markedly  increased  hematopoietic  activity.  The  increased  hematopoiesis  in 
these  animals  is  accompanied  by  a  reticulocytosis  of  the  circulating  blood. 
The  cause  of  increased  hematopoiesis  in  these  animals  has  not  been  demon- 
strated. 

j.  Lymphocijte  counts.  After  an  alarming  stimulus  there  is  an  immediate 
rise  in  the  number  of  circulating  lymphocytes,  which  is  then  followed  by  a 
characteristic  lymphopenia  and  eventually  a  return  to  normal  levels.  The 
initial  rise  in  circulating  lymphocytes  probably  is  at  least  in  part  due  to 
release  of  lymphocj-tes  from  lymphoid  tissues  in  response  to  an  initial  re- 
lease of  epinephrine,  but  this  release  by  no  means  accounts  for  the  entire 
rise  (Gordon,  1955).  The  prolonged  phase  of  lymphopenia  which  begins  in 
the  early  stages  of  the  increased  pituitary-adrenocortical  activity  probably 
results  from  the  destruction  of  medium  and  small-sized  lymphocytes  within 
the  lymphatic  organs  by  the  corticoids  (Gordon,  1955) .  However,  there  is 
also  some  reason  to  believe  that  lymphocytes  may  migrate  to  depot  situa- 
tions. Over  a  prolonged  period  the  main  cause  of  the  lymphopenia  appears 
to  be  due  to  the  reduced  amount  of  lymphoid  tissue  and  decreased  lympho- 
cytopoiesis  with  a  decreased  delivery  of  lymphocytes  into  the  circulation. 
However,  with  small  amounts  of  adrenal  factors  there  may  be  an  acutal 
hyperplasia  of  lymphoid  elements,  so  that  interpretations  must  be  made 
with  caution. 

Lymphocyte  counts  as  criteria  of  stress  in  wild  mammals  are  subject  to 
the  same  criticism  as  other  measurements  made  on  the  living  mammal :  the 
response  is  rapid  enough  and  the  counts  labile  enough  so  that  there  is 
real  danger  that  counts  may  reflect  alarming  stimuli  induced  by  handling, 
thus  masking  any  other  effects  which  may  be  the  main  point  of  the  study. 


252  /.  J.  Christian 

k.  Eosinophil  counts.  The  numbers  of  circvilating  eosinophils  are  beyond 
doubt  diminished  by  adrenal  factors  and  eosinophil  counts  are  commonly 
used  to  assess  the  response  of  the  adrenal  cortex  to  ACTH  or  indicate  in- 
creased pituitary-adrenocortical  activity  (Speirs  and  Meyer,  1949;  Speirs, 
1955).  The  reduction  in  eosinophils  appears  to  result  mainly  from  their 
increased  destruction  under  the  influence  of  cortical  hormones.  A  reduction 
in  eosinophils  can  be  effected  by  epinephrine  as  well  as  by  cortical  hormones, 
and  there  is  evidence  that  the  presence  of  cortical  hormone  is  required  for 
epinephrine  to  produce  an  eosinopenia  (Gordon,  1955).  It  is  mainly  with 
respect  to  whether  or  not  cortical  hormones  are  necessary  for  the  eosino- 
penic  action  of  epinephrine  that  the  specificity  of  eosinopenia  as  an  indica- 
tor of  increased  adrenocortical  activity  has  been  questioned.  Nevertheless, 
if  epinephrine  produces  a  marked  eosinopenia  in  the  intact  animal,  eosino- 
phil counts  in  mammals,  especially  wild,  appear  to  have  limited  value. 
Fear  resulting  from  handling,  trapping,  etc.,  not  only  would  produce 
eosinopenia,  but  could  produce  it  without  necessarily  having  an  increase 
in  adrenocortical  activity.  Nevertheless  Louch  (1958)  used  eosinophil 
counts  to  assess  adrenocortical  function  in  relation  to  changes  in  popula- 
tions of  voles  and  with  adequate  precautions  the  use  of  eosinophil  counts 
for  assessing  adrenocortical  activity  in  natural  populations  seems  to  have 
considerable  value  in  these  experiments.  However  it  is  not  possible  to 
state  definitely  whether  the  declines  in  eosinophils  were  due  to  cortical  or 
medullary  hormones,  although  it  seems  logical  that  the  former  were  respon- 
sible and  there  was  cortical  hypertrophy.  Southwick  (1959)  has  also  related 
declines  in  eosinophil  counts  in  mice  to  increased  adrenocortical  functions, 
but  again  the  role  of  epinephrine  has  not  been  evaluated.  Eosinophils  de- 
cline about  an  hour  after  acute  stimulation  of  the  pituitary-adrenocortical 
systems  (Louch,  1958) ;  therefore  using  counts  as  criteria  of  cortical  func- 
tion with  respect  to  chronic  stimuli  has  many  inherent  hazards.  Acute  re- 
sponses to  handling,  fear,  or  line  trapping  could  easily  mask  any  effects  pro- 
duced by  the  chronic  stimulus  of  changing  population  density.  The  relative 
roles  of  epinephrine  and  of  the  cortical  hormones  in  producing  the  observed 
declines  in  eosinophils  following  emotional  stimuli  in  mice  must  be  assessed 
before  one  can  state  with  finality  that  the  cause  was  increased  cortical 
activity. 

I.  Neutrophil  counts.  A  rise  in  circulating  neutrophils  accompanies  in- 
creased pituitary-adrenocortical  activity,  but  similar  changes  can  be  in- 
duced by  so  many  factors  that,  even  though  they  may  involve  increased 
adrenocortical  secretion,  neutrophil  counts  are  not  very  useful  indices 
with  which  to  measure  pituitary-adrenal  activity. 

m.  Liver  glycogen.  The  accumulation  of  glycogen  by  the  liver  has  been 
used  as  a  means  of  assaying  various  steroids  for  their  activity  on  carbohy- 


2.  Endocrines  and  Populations  253 

drate  metabolism  in  adrenalectomized  rats  (Dorfman,  1949;  Ingle,  1950). 
This  response  is  a  reflection  of  the  ability  of  adrenal  corticoids  to  stimulate 
gluconeogenesis.  Glycogen  in  the  liver  can  be  measured  chemically  (Marto- 
rano,  1957)  or  can  be  visualized  histochemically  by  the  appropriate  proce- 
dures (Dempsey,  1948;  Lillie,  1954).  Liver  glycogen  presumably  falls  as  a 
result  of  exhaustion  of  the  animal's  ability  to  adapt  (Frank,  1953) .  Exactly 
what  becomes  exhausted  under  these  circumstances  is  problematical,  but  it 
apparently  is  not  the  ability  of  the  adrenal  cortex  to  secrete  steroids  (Rosen- 
feld,  1958) .  Nevertheless,  it  is  true  that  blood  glucose  and  liver  glycogen 
eventually  fall  to  extremely  low  levels  following  intense  and  prolonged 
stimulation  by  alarming  stimuli.  This  result  may  follow  prolonged  stimula- 
tion of  the  sympatho-adrenal  system.  Exhaustion  of  glycogen,  consequently 
of  glucose  reserves,  has  been  observed  in  natural  populations  of  snowshoe 
hares  (Green  and  Larson,  1938;  Green  et  al,  1939)  and  voles  (Frank,  1953) 
during  episodes  of  mass  mortality  and  may  possibly  be  explained  on  the 
basis  of  exhaustion  of  the  adaptive  reserves. 

A  word  of  caution  should  be  inserted  on  the  use  of  liver  glycogen  as  an 
indicator  or  activity  of  the  carbohydrate-active  corticoids.  Under  carefully 
controlled  conditions  in  the  laboratory  such  measurements  are  very  useful, 
but  they  do  require  rigid  controls  and  precise,  highly  standardized  proce- 
dures. Martorano  (1957)  has  studied  the  variables  having  important  effects 
on  liver  glycogen.  The  amount  of  time  between  killing  and  enzymatic 
immobilization,  the  manner  of  killing,  and  a  variety  of  other  factors  can 
alter  glycogen  levels.  Glycogen  and  glucose  levels  in  the  liver  decline  rapidly 
after  death.  There  also  is  a  daily  cycle  in  the  levels  of  liver  glycogen  which 
is  associated  with  feeding  and  activity  (Martorano,  1957).  Therefore,  re- 
ported liver  glycogen  levels  must  be  critically  examined  in  relation  to  the 
procedures  used  and  their  reliability. 

n.  Changes  in  the  digestive  organs  and  gastrointestinal  tract.  Alarming 
stimuli,  ACTH,  or  cortisone  produce  a  marked  loss  of  the  acidophilic 
zymogen  granules  and  basophilic  cytoplasmic  pentose  nucleic  acids  from 
the  acinar  cells  of  the  pancreas  and  serous  salivary  gland  cells  (Ehrich  and 
Seifter,  1948;  Selye,  1950).  The  pituitary  adrenocortical  system  also  pro- 
foundly affects  the  function  of  the  mucosa  of  the  stomach  and  intestinal 
tract  (Baker  and  Abrams,  1954;  Baker  and  Bridgman,  1954;  Gray  and 
Ramsey,  1957).  The  adrenocortical  hormone  apparently  stimulates  the 
secretion  of  pepsinogen  by  gastric  zymogenic  cells  (Gray  and  Ramsey, 
1957;  Mason,  1959),  which  is  reflected  morphologically  by  a  loss  of  the 
intracellular  pepsinogen  granules  and  increased  cytoplasmic  basophilia 
(Baker  and  Bridgman,  1954).  Additional  changes  may  not  be  noted  histo- 
logically in  the  mucosal  cells  of  the  intestine  (Baker  and  Bridgman,  1954). 


254  /.  /.   Christian 

3.  Generalized  Effects  of  Physiologic  Adaptation 

a.  Growth.  Associated  with  acute  adaptation  there  is  usually  a  suppres- 
sion of  growth  (Selye,  1950) .  Selye  (1950)  has  suggested  that  during  actua- 
tion of  the  pituitary-adrenocortical  system  there  is  a  decreased  production 
of  the  other  hormones  of  the  adenohypophysis,  including  growth  hormone. 
However,  injected  corticoids  or  ACTH  into  intact  animals  also  results  in 
suppression  of  growth  and  ACTH  will  inhibit  growth  produced  by  growth 
hormone  in  h^Tpophysectomized  rats  (Jones,  1957).  It  was  pointed  out 
earlier  in  this  chapter  that  the  adrenal  carbohydrate-active  corticoids  in- 
crease protein  catabolism,  prevent  the  gro^^iih  of  bone,  lymphoid  tissue, 
skin,  connective  tissue  with  the  production  of  collagen,  and  block  mitoses 
in  general.  Therefore  the  suppression  of  growth  by  alarming  stimuli  may 
reflect  decreased  secretion  of  growth  hormone  as  well  as  the  direct  suppre- 
sive  action  of  the  carbohydrate-action  corticoids  on  mitoses  and  therefore 
growth  and  development.  Diminished  thyroid  activity  during  periods  of 
actively  increased  adrenocortical  activity  may  also  play  a  role  in  this  phe- 
nomenon. Selye  (1950)  has  pointed  out  that  most  of  the  observed  changes 
following  an  alarmmg  stimulus  serve  to  maintain  life  and  that  other,  less 
immediately  important,  functions  are  suppressed. 

b.  Inflammation  and  granulation.  The  adrenocorticoids,  cortisone,  hydro- 
cortisone, and  to  a  lesser  extent  corticosterone,  exert  powerful  anti- 
inflammatory effects  which  stem  from  the  suppression  of  growth  of  connec- 
tive tissue,  the  depression  of  Ij'mphocyte  activity,  and  interference  with 
the  phagocj^tic  process  (Dougherty,  1953;  Robinson  and  Smith,  1953; 
Thomas,  1953),  but  in  addition  they  prevent  the  mobilization  of  all  of  the 
usual  elements  of  an  inflammatory  response  around  the  site  of  injury 
(Taubenhaus  and  Amromin,  1950;  Dougherty,  1953;  Robinson  and  Smith, 
1953;  Gordon,  1955;  Dougherty  and  Schneebeli,  1955).  Androgens  and 
estrogens  potentiate  these  anti-inflammatory  responses.  They  decrease  the 
destruction  of  fibroblasts  and  the  invasion  of  polymorphonuclear  leuco- 
cytes and  macrophages.  The  appearance  of  epithelioid  macrophages,  giant 
cells,  and  formation  of  new  fibroblasts  and  macrophages  are  suppressed  by 
cortisone,  either  injected  or  implanted  as  pellets  (Baker,  1954) .  Dougherty 
and  Schneebeli  (1955)  explain  the  inhibition  of  the  inflammatory  response 
around  the  site  of  injury  in  the  following  way:  When  there  is  cellular  injury, 
substances  are  released  from  the  injured  cells  which  trigger  a  series  of  re- 
sponses which  comprise  inflammation  (Menkin,  1955;  Dougherty  and 
Schneebeli,  1955).  Cortisone  or  hydrocortisone  inhibit  the  inflammatory 
response  by  protecting  the  surviving  cells  from  the  actions  of  the  released 
products  of  cellular  injury.  Growth  hormone,  deoxycorticosterone,  and 
aldosterone  appear  to  exert  a  stimulating  effect  on  inflarmnation  and  the 


2.  Endocrines  and  Populations  255 

development  of  granulation  tissue  (Selye,  1955;  Dougherty  and  Schneebeli, 
1955) .  These  hormones,  however,  also  inhibit  the  anti-inflammatory  action 
of  cortisone,  hydrocortisone,  and  corticosterone  and  may  enhance  the  local 
inflammatory  response  by  increasing  the  susceptibility  of  the  cells  to  the 
inflaming  stimulus  (Dougherty  and  Schneebeli,  1955).  Therefore,  if  there 
is  a  reduction  in  the  secretion  of  growth  hormone  with  a  simultaneous  in- 
crease in  the  production  of  ACTH  and  the  adrenocortical  steroids,  there 
will  not  only  be  a  direct  suppression  of  the  inflammatory  responses  to  infec- 
tion or  injury,  but  also  a  withdrawal  of  the  factors  which  ordinarily  would 
stimulate  such  activity.  The  effects  of  various  alarming  stimuli  or  hormones 
on  inflammation  and  granulation  in  the  intact  animal  have  been  studied 
and  measured  by  using  experimental  granulomas  (Meier  et  at,  1950;  Selye 
and  Bois,  1954;  Robert  and  Nezamis,  1957;  Christian  and  Williamson, 
1958)  or  other  means  of  inducing  inflammation  and  granulation.  It  has  been 
shown  that  in  addition  to  suppressing  inflammation  the  carbohydrate- 
active  corticoids  also  suppress  the  formation  of  granulation  tissue  and 
healing,  primarily  by  preventing  connective  tissue  growth.  There  can  be 
little  doubt  that  the  normal  defenses  against  infection  are  severely  depressed 
in  stressed  animals. 

c.  Antibody  formation.  Antibodies  are  formed  mainly  in  the  lymphatic 
tissues  (Kenning  and  Van  der  Slikke,  1950;  Kass  et  al.,  1953a;  Kelsall  and 
Crabb,  1958) .  One  school  maintains  that  this  function  resides  primarily  in 
the  plasma  cells,  w^hile  another  group  holds  that  lymphatic  cells  in  general 
are  capable  of  manufacturing  antibodies  (Kenning  and  Van  der  Slikke, 
1950;  Dougherty,  1953;  Kelsall  and  Crabb,  1958).  We  do  not  intend  to 
enter  into  this  controversy  at  the  present  time,  but  it  seems  relatively 
certain  that  the  lymphoid  tissues  are  primarily  responsible  for  the  produc- 
tion of  antibodies.  A  variety  of  experiments  have  shown  that  injected 
corticoids  or  the  increased  secretion  of  endogenous  corticoids,  brought 
about  either  by  injected  ACTH  or  in  response  to  alarming  stimuli,  markedly 
suppress  the  formation  of  antibodies  (Kass  et  al.,  1953a) .  Protein  manufac- 
ture, and  therefore  the  formation  of  antibodies,  requires  the  presence  of 
nucleic  acids  in  the  cells,  especially  in  the  cytoplasm,  and  antibody  forma- 
tion is  normally  associated  with  an  increase  in  nucleic  acid  content  of  the 
lymphoid  organs  (Kass  et  al.,  1953a;  Kelsall  and  Crabb,  1958).  Therefore, 
when  there  is  interference  with  nucleic  acid  metabolism  or  its  formation, 
there  is  an  accompanying  reduction  in  the  rate  of  formation  of  antibodies 
(Kass  et  al.,  1953a;  Kelsall  and  Crabb,  1958).  We  have  seen  that  in  addi- 
tion to  actually  destroying  lymphoid  tissue,  the  glucocorticoids  reduce  the 
PNA  content  of  the  remaining  lymphatic  cells.  The  ability  of  the  reticulo- 
endothelial system,  and  possibly  other  cells,  to  dispose  of  phagocytized 
particulate  material  is  also  impaired  (Thomas,  1953)  even  though  phago- 


256  /.  /.   Christian 

cytosis  may  be  stimulated  (Gordon  and  Katsh,  1949;  Thomas,  1953).  By 
these  several  mechanisms  the  production  of  antibodies  may  be  seriously 
impaired  following  activation  of  the  adrenal  cortex  although  there  appears 
to  be  a  dose-response  relationship  (Dougherty,  1953;  Dougherty  and 
Schneebeli,  1955).  While  most  of  these  effects  were  demonstrated  most 
clearly  by  injecting  adrenocortical  hormones,  the  same  effects  have  been 
shown  repeatedly  following  stimulation  of  adrenocortical  secretion  in  the 
intact  animal. 

d.  Resistance  to  infection.  The  three  immediately  preceding  topics  all 
deal  with  factors  involved  in  the  resistance  to  infection.  It  stands  to  reason 
that  reducing  the  inflammatory  response  to,  and  depressing  the  formation 
of  antibodies  against,  infectious  agents  will  inevitably  impair  the  ability  of 
an  animal  to  resist  infection.  Cortisone,  hydrocortisone,  and  ACTH  have 
been  shown  to  decrease  resistance  to  a  variety  of  experimental  infections 
caused  by  a  variety  of  infectious  agents  including  streptococcal,  pneumo- 
coccal, tuberculosis  infections  in  mice,  rats,  and  guinea  pigs,  brucellosis, 
malaria  in  monkeys,  and  others  (Kligman  etal.,  1951 ;  Schmidt  and  Squires, 
1951 ;  Selye,  1951 ;  Kass  et  al.,  1953b;  Le  Maistre  et  al.,  1953;  Robinson  and 
Smith,  1953).  The  pathogenicity  of  various  agents  has  been  increased  by 
cortisone  injection.  For  example,  the  virulence  of  Coxsackie  infections  in 
mice  was  greatly  enhanced  by  cortisone  (Boring  et  al.,  1955),  and  polio- 
myelitis can  be  made  a  paralytic  disease  in  the  normally  resistant  hamster 
by  cortisone  or  hydrocortisone  (Shwartzman  and  Aronson,  1953).  Vire- 
mias  may  likewise  be  prolonged  appreciably  by  the  adrenal  glucocorticoids 
(Whitney  and  Anigstein,  1953;  Pollard  and  Wilson,  1955).  The  list  of 
experimental  infections  which  have  been  made  more  virulent,  prolonged, 
or  otherwise  increased  in  their  pathogenicity  by  treatment  with  carbohy- 
drate-active adrenocorticoids  or  ACTH  (Selye,  1951)  is  long,  and  there  is 
no  point  in  listing  them  in  detail  here. 

Whenever  experiments  with  injected  hormones  are  considered  the  ques- 
tion arises  whether  or  not  the  same  events  may  occur  as  a  result  of  in- 
creased endogenous  secretion  of  the  same  or  similar  hormones.  A  criticism 
frequently  made  of  experiments  with  exogenous  hormones,  especially  with 
large  doses,  is  that  the  results  are  pharmacologic  rather  than  physiologic. 
However,  it  is  by  using  isolated  hormones  in  highly  controlled  situations 
that  an  understanding  of  the  basic  mechanisms  is  gained.  Nevertheless, 
before  one  can  extrapolate  from  these  data  to  natural  events,  comparable 
changes  must  be  shown  to  occur  in  natural  or  seminatural  conditions. 
Changes  in  host  resistance  may  result  from  adverse  environmental  stimuli, 
possibly  as  a  result  of  adrenocortical  activity.  It  has  long  been  common 
knowledge  that  excess  fatigue,  chilling,  and  a  variety  of  comparable  stimuli 
increase  the  susceptibility  of  humans  to  colds  and  other  infections.  It  should 


2.  Endocrines  and  Populations  257 

be  apparent  now  that  most  of  these  same  stimuH  also  increase  the  secretion 
of  adrenocortical  steroids.  However,  commonly  accepted  truisms  still  do 
not  constitute  experimental  evidence  and  proof  of  such  conclusions,  but 
several  experiments  have  shown  that  host  resistance  is  decreased  by  ex- 
posing the  animals  to  stimuli  which  are  known  to  increase  adrenocortical 
activity.  When  mice  are  exposed  to  4°  C.  for  a  period  of  time,  Coxsackie 
infections  become  much  more  pathogenic,  spreading  especially  to  the  heart 
and  liver  in  adult  mice  (Boring  et  al.,  1956) .  Duninished  resistance  of  mice 
to  trichinosis  and  to  tuberculosis  has  been  demonstrated  by  procedures 
which  also  produce  increased  pituitary-adrenocortical  activity  and  depress 
inflammation  and  granulation  (Christian  and  Williamson,  1958;  Tobach 
and  Block,  1956;  Davis  and  Read,  1958) .  Some  aspects  of  these  studies  will 
be  considered  in  more  detail  later. 

e.  Reproduction.  Suppression  of  reproduction  is  a  very  important  aspect 
of  the  endocrine  adaptive  responses  (Selye,  1939,  1950).  The  decrease  in 
reproductive  function  is  in  many  ways  a  more  sensitive  measure  of  the 
existence  of  altered  physiologic  functions  in  response  to  adversity  than  the 
increase  in  adrenocortical  activity  and  some  of  its  sequelae  (Christian, 
1955a,  b,  1956,  1959b,  c) .  The  bulk  of  the  experimental  evidence  indicates 
that,  like  growth  hormone,  the  secretion  of  pituitary  gonadotropins  is  sup- 
pressed in  response  to  alarmmg  stimuli  which  evoke  an  increased  secretion 
of  ACTH  (Selye,  1939,  1950;  Christian,  1956,  1959b,  c).  It  is  another 
indication  that  the  immediate  restoration  of  the  normal  internal  environ- 
ment takes  precedence  over  functions  which  are  less  important  to  the  im- 
mediate siu-vival  of  the  individual.  Cold,  heat,  disease,  trauma,  severe 
emotional  stress,  and  other  stimuli  will  depress  normal  reproductive  func- 
tions (Marrian  and  Parkes,  1929;  Selye,  1939;  Bohanan,  1939;  Poindexter, 
1949;  Denison  and  Zarrow,  1955;  Barnett  and  Manly,  1956;  Christian  and 
LeMunyan,  1958;  Christian,  1959b).  The  suppression  of  gonadotropin 
secretion  is  evidently  the  primary  cause  of  the  inhibition  of  reproductive 
function  (Mulinos  et  al,  1939,  Selye,  1950;  Srebnik  et  al,  1958),  although 
there  may  be  direct  effects  of  increased  adrenocortical  activity  and  altered 
thyroid  function,  as  mentioned  earlier  (Brynes  and  Shipley,  1950;  Baker 
et  al,  1950;  Smith,  1951;  Brimblecombe  et  al,  1954).  The  secretion  of 
gonadotropins  seems  to  be  regulated  principally  by  the  hypothalamus 
(Anderson  and  Haymaker,  1948a,  b;  Markee  et  al,  1952;  Everett  and 
Sawyer,  1953;  Hammond,  1954;  Critchlow  and  Sawyer,  1955;  Nalbandov 
et  al,  1955;  Laqueur  et  al,  1955;  Fortier,  1957;  Greer,  1957;  D'Angelo  and 
Traum,  1958  Everett,  1959) ,  although  there  is  evidence  that  the  secretion 
of  luteotropin  by  the  pituitary  may  be  independent  of  the  hypothalamus 
(Everett,  1956).  The  hypothalamic  centers  involved  in  the  regulation  of 
the  secretion  of  the  gonadotropins  apparently  are  distinct  from  those 


258  /.  /.   Christian 

responsible  for  the  regulation  of  ACTH  and  TSH  secretion  (Laqueur  et  al., 
1955;  Greer,  1957;  D'Angelo  and  Traum,  1958).  In  addition  there  are 
neurogenic  factors  involved  in  ovulation  which  consist  of  adrenergic  and 
neurogenic  components  (Markee  et  al.,  1952;  Everett  and  Sawyer,  1953; 
Nalbandov  et  al,  1955)  and  probably  also  in  the  milk  let-down  reflex 
(Grosvenor  and  Turner,  1959a).  Finally,  as  in  other  endocrine  regulating 
mechanisms,  the  level  of  circulating  sex  steroids  seems  to  exert  a  regulating 
effect  on  the  release  of  the  particular  gonadotropin  responsible  for  their 
release  (Selye,  1947;  Sturgis,  1950;  Byrnes  and  Shipley,  1950;  Byrnes  and 
Meyer,  1951).  Whether  these  steroids  exert  their  effects  on  the  hypothala- 
mus or  on  the  anterior  pituitary  itself  is  not  known.  In  any  event,  with- 
drawal of  stimulation  by  the  gonadotropins  leads  to  atrophy  of  the  gonads 
and  decline  in  the  production  of  their  respective  steroids  (Burrows,  1949). 
The  decline  in  the  production  of  sex  steroids  is  in  turn  followed  by  atrophy 
of  those  accessory  organs  and  secondary  sex  characteristics  which  depend 
on  the  sex  steroids  for  their  activity  and  maintenance  (Burrows,  1949). 

If  a  male  animal  is  subjected  to  adverse  circumstances  for  any  length  of 
time,  there  is  a  marked  decrease  in  spermatogenesis  and  a  reduction  in  the 
secretion  of  androgens  as  indicated  by  atrophy  of  the  seminal  vesicles, 
prostate,  and  preputial  glands  (Christian,  1956;  1959b;  Christian  and 
LeMunyan,  1958) .  These  changes  may  all  be  followed  by  the  changes  in 
their  weights,  but  weight  changes  should  be  assessed  by  appropriately 
selected  histologic  examination  of  the  organs.  In  young  animals  there  may 
be  either  a  delay  in  the  onset  of  puberty  or  a  total  suppression  of  the  de- 
velopment of  puberty,  as  indicated  by  the  development  of  spermatogenesis 
and  the  sex  accessories  or,  in  the  mouse,  by  a  failure  of  the  X-zone  to 
involute  at  the  usual  time  (Christian,  1956,  1959a,  b).  If  mature  animals 
are  subjected  to  severe  stimuli,  there  may  be  easily  discernible  degenerative 
changes  in  the  tubules  of  the  testes  and  cells  of  the  spermatogenic  series. 

Changes  in  the  female  reproductive  function  are  less  easily  seen  than  in 
the  male,  as  organ  weights  do  not  provide  as  useful  a  criterion  of  changes. 
Nevertheless,  changes  in  estrus  are  usually  evident  and  can  be  detected  by 
vaginal  perforation  or  by  vaginal  smears.  Depending  on  the  severity  of  the 
inducing  stimulus,  estrus  may  be  prolonged  or  totally  suppressed.  Uterine 
weight  may  reflect  reproductive  suppression,  and  would  be  especially 
valuable  in  demonstrating  a  delay  in  the  onset  of  puberty  in  female  animals. 
In  some  circumstances  there  may  be  a  total  suppression  of  reproductive 
activity.  It  is  in  actual  reproductive  performance  that  the  effects  of  sup- 
pression of  the  reproductive  endocrine  system  becomes  most  evident.  De- 
pending on  the  severity  of  the  stimulus  and  on  the  species  involved,  there 
may  be  a  complete  failure  to  become  pregnant,  which  may  be  due  to  a 
failure  of  ovulation  or  a  failure  of  the  shed  ova  to  implant  in  the  uterus  or, 


2.  Endocrines  and  Populations  259 

if  the  females  do  become  pregnant,  there  may  be  a  marked  increase  in  fetal 
mortality  with  increased  resorption  of  the  embryos  (Christian,  1959a,  b) . 

The  causes  of  fetal  mortality  in  these  circumstances  have  not  been 
explored  in  detail,  but  a  number  of  factors  may  be  involved.  It  is  well 
known  that  cortisone  and  hydrocortisone  have  serious  effects  on  the  fetus, 
apparently  by  inhibiting  growth  and  development  (Glaubach,  1952;  Fraser 
et  al,  1953;  Davis  and  Plotz,  1954;  Kalter,  1954).  The  resultant  defect 
probably  depends  to  a  large  extent  on  the  developmental  stage  of  the  fetus 
when  these  hormones  are  active,  as  is  the  case  with  nutritional  deficiencies 
(Lutwak-Mann,  1958) .  A  variety  of  congenital  defects  have  been  produced 
experimentally  by  the  injection  of  these  hormones.  However,  high  doses  are 
required  to  produce  these  effects;  furthermore,  with  chronic  injection  of 
cortisone  some  fetuses  do  not  seem  to  be  affected  (Seifter  et  al,  1951). 
Hydrocortisone  and  cortisone  increase  the  incidence  of  fetal  mortality  in 
rats  during  the  second  half  of  pregnancy.  The  mechanism  is  unknown,  but 
it  has  been  suggested  that  it  may  be  due  in  part  to  premature  ''aging"  of 
the  vasculature  of  the  placenta  (Seifter  et  al,  1951).  Increased  mortality 
may  reflect  fatal  defects  in  the  growth  of  the  embryos  due  to  the  action  of 
these  hormones.  In  other  words,  there  may  be  time-dosage  relationships 
which  determine  whether  the  effects  of  these  glucocorticoids  will  be  fatal  or 
will  result  in  "congenital  defects,"  such  as  cleft  palate  or  cardiac  anomalies. 
However,  these  explanations  leave  unanswered  the  question  why  many 
embryos  subjected  to  the  same  influences  are  born  viable  and  free  of  de- 
fects, even  from  the  same  pregnancies.  However,  there  are  many  other 
factors  to  consider  when  discussing  intrauterine  mortality  resulting  from 
activation  of  physiological  adaptive  systems.  One  must  consider  the  in- 
creased secretion  of  adrenal  androgens  by  those  species  in  which  androgens 
or  proandrogens  constitute  a  major  secretory  product.  These  compounds 
possibly  may  directly  inhibit  the  action  of  estrogens  and  progestins.  For 
example,  it  has  been  shown  that  testosterone  can  completely  inhibit  the 
feminizing  action  of  estrogens  on  developing  rat  embryos  (Greene  et  al, 
1941).  More  importantly,  the  adrenal  androgens  can  inhibit  the  secretion 
of  gonadotropins,  especially  of  FSH  (Byrnes  and  Shipley,  1950;  Byrnes 
and  Meyer,  1951 ;  Dorfman  and  Shipley,  1956).  Whether  the  decline  in  the 
secretion  of  gonadotropins  with  increased  ACTH  secretion  is  due  to  inhibi- 
tion by  increased  circulatory  levels  of  adrenal  androgens  or  whether  it  is 
independent  of  the  androgens,  cannot  be  said.  However,  it  seems  probable 
that  the  androgens  are  not  important  or,  if  so,  only  in  a  limited  number  of 
species.  The  adrenal  secretion  of  androgens  or  their  precursors  in  many 
species  does  not  appear  to  be  sufficient  to  account  for  the  effects  seen.  For 
example,  in  humans  pituitary  blockage  requires  much  higher  doses  of 
testosterone  than  is  required  to  produce  overt  androgenic  responses.  Very 


260  J'  J'  Christian 

likely  the  reciprocal  relationship  between  ACTH  and  gonadotropin  secre- 
tion is  independent  of  the  adrenal  androgens  in  most  species,  although  the 
latter  possibly  may  enhance  a  pre-existing  inhibition  of  the  gonadotropins. 
However,  they  may  be  of  considerable  importance  in  house  mice,  rats,  and 
other  species  which  secrete  appreciable  amounts  of  adrenal  androgens. 
There  undoubtedly  are  many  other  factors  contributing  to  intra-uterine 
mortality  which  have  not  been  discussed  here.  In  general,  however,  it  is 
evident  that  factors  increasing  pituitary-adrenocortical  activity  are  asso- 
ciated with  increased  intra-uterine  mortality,  although  the  details  of  the 
mechanisms  are  largely  unknown. 

Finally,  if  the  young  are  born,  there  may  be  a  failure  of  lactation  due  to 
deficiency  in  the  hormones  normally  required  for  the  maintenance  of 
adequate  lactation  (Christian  and  LeMunyan,  1958).  The  changes  in  re- 
productive function  following  inanition  have  been  show^n  to  be  due  pri- 
marily to  a  decrease  in  the  secretion  of  gonadotropins,  as  the  gonads  remain 
responsive  to  injected  gonadotrophin  (Srebnik  et  al.,  1958).  Most  of  these 
alterations  in  reproductive  function  will  be  discussed  in  more  detail  later. 

/.  Acute  visceral  degenerative  changes.  Acute  degenerative  changes  of  the 
liver,  pancreas,  and  parotid  glands  have  been  described  as  occurring  during 
an  alarm  reaction  to  a  variety  of  stimuli  (Selye,  1950;  Ehrich  and  Seifter, 
1948).  Selye  (1950)  states  that  similar  changes  also  occur  during  the 
"exhaustion"  phase  of  adaptation. 

The  most  constant  hepatic  changes  are  pycnosis,  cloudy  swelling,  and 
stromal  edema.  Occasionally  there  is  fatty  infiltration,  severe  atrophy,  focal 
necrosis,  and  leucocytic  infiltration.  However,  many  of  these  changes  are 
duplicated  by  a  variety  of  specific  stimuli  and  cannot  be  considered  as 
diagnostic  in  any  sense  unless  all  other  possible  factors,  other  than  increased 
pituitary-adrenocortical  activity,  have  been  ruled  out. 

The  salivary  glands,  especially  the  parotid,  also  exhibit  degeneration  and 
necrosis  of  the  parenchyma,  often  with  suppurative  inflammation,  during 
an  alarm  reaction  (Ehrich  and  Seifter,  1948).  In  addition  to  the  zymogen 
discharge  already  alluded  to,  the  pancreatic  parenchyma  may  exhibit  focal 
necrosis  and  inflammation  comparable  to  that  seen  in  the  salivary  glands 
(Selye,  1950).  The  cytoplasmic  nucleic  acids  are  markedly  decreased 
during  acute  adrenocortical  stimulation  or  in  response  to  injected  adreno- 
corticoids,  and  may  precede  the  degenerative  changes.  Loss  of  cytoplasmic 
basophilia  in  hepatic  cells  under  the  same  circumstances  reflects  the  same 
kind  of  influence  of  adrenal  steroids  on  cytoplasmic  nucleic  acids. 

g.  Other  effects.  In  addition  to  those  effects  of  physiologic  adaptation 
which  have  been  discussed,  there  are  others  which  stem  from  a  general  shift 
in  physiologic  functions  or  as  yet  have  not  been  related  to  the  activity  of 
any  specific  hormone.  Among  these  is  enlargement  of  the  intervertebral 


2.  Endocrines  and  Populations  261 

discs  (nucleus  pulposus)  which  has  been  found  in  voles  {Microtus  orcaden- 
sis)  subjected  to  emotional  stress  (Chitty  et  al,  1956) .  The  enlargement  of 
the  intervertebral  discs  could  result  from  altered  fluid  and  electrolyte 
balances  brought  about  by  increased  activity  of  adrenal  corticoids,  or 
might  follow  alterations  in  the  ground  substance  by  cortical  hormones; 
however,  these  explanations  are  conjectural  as  the  mechanism  is  as  yet 
unknown. 

Physiologic  adaptation  may  be  accompanied  by  a  variety  of  shifts  in  the 
composition  of  the  blood  with  changes  in  hematocrit,  electrolyte  concentra- 
tions, and  various  metabolites.  Blood  lipids  and  sugar  may  also  shift  mark- 
edly, as  we  have  mentioned  earlier.  However,  these  factors  are  discussed 
in  considerable  detail  elsewhere  (e.g.,  Selye,  1950;  Hartman  and  Brownell, 
1949;  Jones,  1957)  and  will  not  be  considered  further  in  this  discussion. 


Part  2.     Physiologic  Adaptation  and 
Mammalian  Populations 

I.  Introduction 

The  foregoing  account  dealt  largely  with  the  basic  endocrine  and  other 
physiologic  adaptive  mechanisms  that  serve  to  maintain  physiologic  homeo- 
stasis in  the  face  of  a  variety  of  stimuli  tending  to  alter  the  internal  environ- 
ment and  to  equip  the  animal  to  meet  the  demands  of  emergency  situations. 
In  general,  any  stimulus  which  imposes  physiological  demands  on  an  organ- 
ism beyond  those  ordinarily  met  in  undisturbed  idyllic  daily  life  calls  into 
play  a  series  of  feedback  mechanisms  that  regulate  the  secretion  of  hor- 
mones responsible  for  the  maintenance  of  a  relatively  constant  internal 
environment.  These  mechanisms  act  upon  the  distribution  of  the  internal 
environment  via  the  circulatory  channels,  the  composition  of  the  internal 
environment  with  respect  to  fluids,  electrolytes,  glucose,  fats,  and  a  variety 
of  other  metabolites  and  metabolic  products,  and  on  the  supply  of  readily 
available  nutrients  and  oxygen,  especially  for  the  skeletal  muscle  and  ner- 
vous system.  We  have  seen  that  these  effects  are  not  achieved  withovit 
sacrificing  functions  less  immediately  vital  to  the  individual,  such  as  re- 
production, growth,  and  resistance  to  infectious  disease  and  parasitism. 
We  also  have  pointed  out  the  error  in  thinking  that  the  responses  to  all 
adverse  stimuli  are  the  same  and  that  all  necessarily  are  associated  with 
increased  secretion  of  adrenal  carbohydrate-active  corticoids. 


262  /.  /.   Christian 

The  mechanisms,  hormonal  effects,  and  general  responses  so  far  described 
have  been  confined  largely  to  the  results  of  experiments  in  the  laboratory 
with  the  usual  laboratory  species.  Very  little  work  has  been  done  on  native 
mammals  in  the  laboratory,  and  comparative  studies  are  certainly  needed. 
In  addition,  the  work  has  been  to  a  great  extent  limited  to  studies  of  the 
effects  of  injected  hormones  or  of  subjecting  laboratory  animals  to  ex- 
tremely severe  conditions. 

The  role  that  physiological  adaptive  mechanisms  play  under  natural 
conditions  was  not  investigated  in  most  of  these  studies.  However,  it  was 
postulated  in  1950  that  these  same  responses  could  be  evoked  by  increased 
population  density  and  that  these  same  physiological  reactions  could  serve 
as  a  feedback  to  regulate  the  growth  of  mammalian  populations,  their 
declines,  and  the  mass  mortality  which  occasionally  terminates  the  build-up 
of  a  natural  population  to  extremely  high  densities  (Christian,  1950b).  It 
was  first  suggested  that  the  intraspecific  strife  and  social  competition  that 
force  animals  into  adverse  circumstances,  together  with  all  of  the  other 
adversities  which  become  aggravated  by  high  population  densities,  would 
elicit  adaptive  responses  such  as  those  which  have  been  described.  There- 
fore one  would  anticipate  a  direct  relationship  between  adrenocortical 
activity  and  a  more  or  less  reciprocal  relationship  between  reproductive 
function  and  population  density.  The  hypothesis  that  physiological  mecha- 
nisms were  active  in  all  populations  in  response  to  changes  in  density  and 
that  most  environmental  deficiencies  acted  through  this  mechanism  was 
inherent  in  the  original  postulate.  This  hypothesis  implied  that  social 
competition  or  pressure  was  the  sole  factor,  always  present  in  all  popula- 
tions, which  could  logically  be  expected  to  elicit  the  gamut  of  adaptive 
responses  in  every  population.  However,  these  relationships  had  to  be 
demonstrated,  and  it  was  necessary  to  show  that  changes  in  population 
alone  could  induce  a  proportional  increase  in  pituitary- adrenocortical 
activity,  decrease  in  reproductive  activity,  decrease  in  resistance  to  disease, 
or  even  death  from  shock,  and  all  the  other  reactions  and  responses  which 
have  been  described  in  the  preceding  section. 

Since  that  time  considerable  evidence  has  been  accumulated  from  the 
laboratory  and  from  natural  populations  which  indicates  that  these  re- 
sponses to  population  density  do  occur  and  that  they  can  regulate  popula- 
tion growth.  The  balance  of  this  chapter  will  be  devoted  to  a  presentation 
of  the  experimental  evidence  for  the  response  of  physiological  adaptive 
mechanisms  to  social  competition  and  therefore  population  density,  and 
the  evidence  implicating  these  mechanisms,  acting  as  a  feedback  system,  in 
the  regulation  of  mammalian  populations.  Finally  there  will  be  a  discussion 
of  the  pertinence  of  this  evidence  to  the  regulation  and  control  of  natural 
populations. 


2.  Endocrines  and  Populations  263 

The  general  plan  of  the  following  discussion  will  be  to  explore  first  the 
ability  of  purely  social  factors  to  affect  adrenocortical  and  reproductive 
function.  Next  the  relation  between  population  density  and  adrenocortical, 
reproductive,  and  other  functions  as  well  as  alterations  in  resistance  to 
disease,  will  be  investigated.  In  general  the  plan  will  proceed  from  popula- 
tions of  fixed  size  in  the  laboratory  to  freely  growing  populations  in  the 
laboratory,  and  finally  to  natural  populations.  Under  each  of  these  experi- 
mental categories  adrenocortical  function,  reproductive  function,  disease 
resistance,  and  mortality  will  be  discussed  with  all  appropriate  experiments 
and  species.  The  effect  of  population  density  on  growth  will  be  discussed 
where  appropriate,  and  the  effects  of  food  and  other  environmental  factors 
on  social  interactions  and  endocrine  function  will  be  discussed.  It  is  not 
possible  to  stay  strictly  within  this  framework,  as  it  is  somewhat  artificial, 
but  it  does  seem  to  offer  the  most  logical  means  of  presenting  the  available 
information  as  it  progresses  from  the  most  artificial  but  most  highly  con- 
trolled experiments  to  natural  populations  which  are  controlled  with  great 
difficulty,  if  at  all. 


II.  Endocrine  Responses  to  Social  Pressures  and  to  Population  Density 

A.    Experiments  in  the  Laboratory  with  Populations  of  Fixed  Size 

One  of  the  basic  tenets  in  the  theory  that  physiological  feedback  mecha- 
nisms can  regulate  population  growth  is  that  a  fundamental  regulating 
factor  must  be  present  and  active  in  all  populations.  Whether  or  not  this 
particular  factor  is  the  proximate  factor  limiting  population  growth  in  a 
given  instance  is  not  important  if  it  is  universally  present.  The  only  known 
element  common  to  all  populations  is  social  interaction,  or  intraspecific 
competition.  Basically,  competition  depends  on  the  behavioral  characteris- 
tic of  the  species,  but  some  sort  of  social  organization  or  mutual  intolerance 
is  exhibited  by  all  species  of  mammals. 

1.  Social  Factors,  Adrenocortical  and  Reproductive  Functions 

Social  interactions  may  arise  from  two  kinds  of  situations:  one  in  which 
there  is  invasion  of  the  private  territory  of  one  animal  by  another;  and 
another  in  which  there  is  conflict  involved  in  the  establishment  and  mainte- 
nance of  a  hierarchical  situation.  The  first  requirement  of  the  hypothesis 
that  physiologic  mechanisms  can  and  do  control  population  growth  is  to 
show  that  purely  behavioral  or  social  interactions,  acting  through  the 
central  nervous  system,  can  induce  endocrine  responses,  especially  of  the 
pituitary-adrenocortical  and  reproductive  systems,  and  to  be  able  to  rule 


264  /.  J.  Christian 

out  other  factors  which  are  known  to  produce  the  observed  changes.  That 
pure  sociopsychologic  factors  can  produce  these  effects  is  clearly  demon- 
strated by  the  following  experiments. 

When  a  strange  vole  {Microtus  agrestis)  of  either  sex  was  placed  daily  for 
27  days  in  a  cage  containing  a  resident  pair  of  voles,  and  therefore  in  what 
was  essentially  their  private  territory,  vicious  fighting  ensued  (Clarke, 
1953).  The  introduced  voles  lost  weight  and  exhibited  a  significant  hyper- 
trophy of  the  adrenals  and  spleens  and  atrophy  of  the  thymus  compared 
to  the  residents.  In  similar  experiments  with  Microtus  orcadensis  there  were 
significant  increases  in  the  size  of  the  livers,  adrenals,  intervertebral  discs, 
and  spleens,  and  decrease  in  the  thymus  in  the  "stressed"  animals,  those 
introduced  as  strangers  to  resident  pairs  (Chitty  et  al.,  1956).  Clarke 
(1953)  attributed  the  changes  in  organ  weight  to  the  actual  fighting, 
stating  that  fighting  is  a  very  effective  form  of  stress  since  it  involves 
vigorous  muscular  exercise  in  addition  to  the  trauma  of  wounds  (Selye, 
1950) .  However,  the  fact  that  the  resident  voles  had  to  fight  as  much  as 
those  which  were  introduced,  and  yet  did  not  reflect  this  with  increased 
adrenocortical  activity,  apparentlj^  was  overlooked.  This  problem  may  be 
explored  by  data  on  albino  mice  (Christian,  1959d).  An  analysis  of  the 
relationship  between  scars  from  fighting  and  adrenal  weight  from  280  male 
albuio  house  mice  from  55  populations  of  4,  5,  or  6  each  showed  that,  al- 
though the  mean  adrenal  weight  of  every  population  increased  appreciably 
with  respect  to  isolated  controls,  there  was  no  relationship  between  the 
amount  of  scarring,  as  an  indication  of  the  severity  of  fighting,  and  adrenal 
weight.  Fm'thermore,  the  presence  or  absence  of  scarring  in  a  population 
made  no  difference  in  adrenal  weight.  The  results  were  similar  irrespective 
of  whether  the  analysis  involved  only  the  differences  between  populations 
or  the  difference  between  individuals.  Adrenal  weight  increased  the  same 
amount  in  populations  in  which  there  was  no  fighting,  or  so  little  that  none 
of  the  mice  had  injuries  from  biting,  as  it  did  in  populations  in  which 
fighting  was  severe  enough  that  most  of  the  animals  were  badly  scarred.  It 
should  be  pointed  out  that  these  injiu'ies  were  superficial  and  for  the  most 
part  represented  bites  through  the  skin  only.  These  results  indicate  quite 
clearly  that  fighting  or  injury  per  se  are  not  the  stimuli  responsible  for 
stimulating  increased  adrenocortical  activity  with  thymic  involution  when 
animals  are  placed  together.  Conclusive  evidence  that  sociopsychologic 
pressures  alone  are  mainly  responsible  for  evoking  these  physiologic  adap- 
tive responses  lies  in  the  following  experimental  results  (Davis  and  Chris- 
tian, 1957) . 

When  house  mice  are  placed  together  in  groups  of  six,  there  is  immediate 
fighting  which  soon  ceases  with  the  establishment  of  a  social  hierarchy  with 
one  mouse  dominant  over  the  others  and  another  subordinate  to  all  the 


2.  Endocrines  and  Populations  265 

others.  The  remaining  mice  arrange  themselves  in  some  sort  of  hierarchy  in 
between.  It  was  found  that  adrenocortical  hypertrophy  was  greatest  in  the 
most  subordinate  animals  and  was  slight  or  absent  in  the  dominant  mice. 
The  adrenals  of  those  in  between  tended  to  fall  in  line  in  between  in  a 
reciprocal  relationship  to  their  dominance  rank.  Fighting  cannot  have  been 
an  important  stimulus  to  mcreased  pituitary-adrenocortical  activity  in 
these  experiments,  as  the  dominant  animals  fought  as  much  as,  or  more 
than,  any  of  the  subordinate  animals.  The  mice  in  these  experiments  also 
exhibited  changes  in  the  weights  of  their  reproductive  organs  consistent 
with  suppressed  secretion  of  gonadotropms  coincident  with  increased 
pituitary-adrenocortical  activity.  There  was  no  clear-cut  decline  in  the 
weights  of  the  reproductive  organs  with  decreasing  social  rank,  but  the 
dominant  mice  had  much  heavier  reproductive  organs,  especially  the  pre- 
putial glands,  then  the  subordinate  animals  (Davis  and  Christian,  1957). 
These  results  were  confirmed  and  intended  in  dogs  by  Eik-nes  (1959),  who 
found  that  the  dominant  dogs  in  groups  secreted  about  half  the  amount 
of  corticoids  that  the  subordinate  dogs  secreted.  Therefore  there  can  be 
little  question  that  there  are  significant  differences  in  adrenocortical  func- 
tion associated  with  differences  in  social  rank. 

In  another  series  of  experiments  Southwick  ( 1959)  demonstrated  that 
moving  mice  into  a  new  environment  daily  could  induce  a  marked  increase 
in  adrenocortical  activity  as  determined  by  eosinophil  counts.  That  the 
eosinopenia  was  not  a  result  of  handling  was  shown  by  the  fact  that  mice 
handled  in  the  same  way  but  not  placed  in  a  strange  situation  responded 
with  only  a  slight  fall  in  circulating  eosinophils.  The  mice  transferred  to  new 
cages  for  a  period  of  time  every  day  adapted  to  the  situation,  as  the  eosino- 
phil count  returned  to  normal  levels  by  the  end  of  the  8-day  experimental 
period.  A  third  series  of  anunals  were  placed  in  groups  of  four  each  day  and 
these  animals  exhibited  an  80%  mean  decline  in  their  eosinophil  counts 
and  these  counts  remained  low  as  long  as  the  animals  were  placed  in  groups. 
Presumably  the  mice  responded  to  grouping  with  a  marked  increase  in 
adrenocortical  activity  and  did  not  adapt  to  the  situation.  It  is  clear  from 
these  results  that  merely  placing  mice  in  a  strange  situation  is  an  emotional 
stimulus  sufficient  to  result  in  a  decline  in  circulating  eosinophils  and  pre- 
sumably in  adrenocortical  activity  and  that  grouping  constitutes  a  more 
profound  stimulus  to  which  animals  fail  to  adapt.  As  we  have  pointed  out 
previously,  it  cannot  be  concluded  finally  that  the  eosinopenia  was  due  to 
increased  adrenocortical  activity,  although  it  seems  likely,  imtil  increased 
secretion  of  epinephrine  is  ruled  out  as  a  causative  factor. 

Similar  results  were  obtained  when  male  Norway  rats  were  placed  in 
groups  (Barnett,  1955) .  The  subordinate  males,  subjected  to  severe  fighting 
for  short  periods,  at  first  showed  marked  decreases  in  adrenocortical  sud- 


266  /.  J-   Christian 

anophilia,  whereas  prolonged  exposure  to  less  severe  fighting  resulted  in 
normal  sudanophilia  with  adrenal  hypertrophy  in  the  subordinate  animals. 
None  of  the  dominant  rats  showed  adrenocortical  hypertrophy  or  changes 
in  sudanophilia  in  spite  of  the  fact  that  they  fought  as  much  or  more  than 
the  subordinate  animals.  Barnett  (1958)  has  recently  published  additional 
evidence  on  a  smaller  number  of  rats  from  which  he  concluded  that  the 
adrenal  cortices  of  both  subordinate  and  dominant  animals  hypertrophy. 
Nevertheless  his  data  show  that  the  mean  adrenal  weight,  relative  or  abso- 
lute, of  subordinate  animals  in  a  group  of  rats  introduced  into  a  colony  was 
appreciably  greater  than  that  of  dominant  animals.  Barnett  used  very  small 
numbers  of  rats  and  based  his  conclusions  on  absolute  rather  than  on  rela- 
tive adrenal  weight,  in  spite  of  the  fact  that  his  animals  varied  from  250  to 
400  gm.  at  the  start  and  170  to  420  gm.  at  the  end  of  the  experiment.  He 
concluded,  on  the  basis  of  thirty  rats,  that  there  was  no  relationship  between 
adrenal  weight  and  body  weight  for  rats  weighing  more  than  150  gm.  This 
conclusion  may  be  ciuestioned  for  several  reasons.  In  the  first  place  the 
mean  adrenal  weight  of  his  animals  increased  with  increasing  body  weight, 
athough  the  differences  were  not  significant.  However,  had  larger  numbers 
of  animals  been  used  it  is  more  than  likely  that  a  significant  increase  in 
adrenal  weight  with  increased  body  weight  could  have  been  shown.  Other 
data  on  over  1200  wild  Norway  rats  shown  conclusively  that  there  is  a 
definite  increase  in  adrenal  weight  with  increasing  body  weights  varying 
from  50  through  600  gm.,  and  that  there  was  a  significant  linear  relationship 
between  the  logarithm  of  the  adrenal  weight  and  body  length  or  weight 
(Christian,  1954;  Christian  and  Davis,  1955) .  Finally,  it  is  well  known  that 
there  is  a  definite  tendency  for  the  larger  animals  to  be  dominant,  therefore 
adrenal  hypertrophy  in  smaller,  subordinate  rats  would  tend  to  make  the 
adrenals  of  dominant  and  subordinate  animals  weigh  the  same.  It  is  of 
considerable  interest,  however,  that  rats  from  colonies  of  mixed  sex  were 
appreciably  larger  than  those  from  all-male  colonies.  In  general  the  amount 
of  sudanophilia  in  the  zona  fasciculata  coincided  with  the  weight  data. 
Nevertheless,  Barnett's  results  appear  to  agree  with  those  established  earlier 
as  well  as  with  the  results  of  the  experiments  of  other  investigators. 

The  above  experiments  warrant  the  general  conclusion  that  in  house  mice 
and  Norway  rats  adrenal  weight  tends  to  be  inversely  related  to  social 
rank  in  that  dominant  animals  exhibit  little  or  no  increase  in  adrenal  weight, 
while  subordinate  animals  show  a  marked  increase.  A  word  of  caution  should 
be  directed  with  regard  to  the  interpretation  of  results  with  adrenal  weights. 
A  hyperactive  adrenal  with  a  loss  of  cortical  lipids  may  weigh  less  than  a 
less  active  gland  containing  a  large  amount  of  lipids.  This  has  been  shown 
to  be  the  case  in  mice  (Christian,  1955a,  1959b)  and  evidently  is  also  true 
in  Barnett's  (1958)  experiments  in  which  the  adrenals  of  his  "interloper" 


2.  Endocrines  and  Populations  267 

rats  weighed  somewhat  less  than  those  of  subordinate  rats,  but  the  adrenals 
of  the  interlopers  contained  little  or  no  lipid. 

The  relationship  between  social  dominance  and  adrenal  activity  is, 
however,  not  as  clear  cut  as  these  experiments  at  first  indicate  and  suggests 
that  a  great  deal  of  work  needs  to  be  done  on  the  factors  which  affect 
dominance-subordinance  relationships  and  w^hat  constitutes  "social  stress" 
in  mammals.  The  adrenals  of  the  dominant  animal  in  a  group  do  not  always 
weigh  the  least  and  occasionally  may  be  the  heaviest,  even  though  the 
average  weight  of  the  adrenals  of  the  dominant  animals  is  appreciably  less 
than  those  of  other  ranks.  Furthermore,  the  amount  of  fighting  varies 
greatly  from  group  to  group.  Finally,  the  amount  of  scarring  on  a  mouse  in 
a  group  is  in  a  general  way  a  measure  of  its  rank.  Observation  has  shown 
that  it  is  generally  safe  to  assume  that  the  unscarred  mouse  in  a  group  is 
the  dominant  animal,  and  yet,  as  we  have  shown,  there  is  no  relationship 
between  the  amount  of  scarring  and  absolute  adrenal  weight  in  groups  of 
albino  male  mice  although  there  was  clearly  a  tendency  for  adrenal  weight 
relative  to  body  weight  to  increase  with  increased  scarring  (Christian, 
1959d) .  These  results  apparently  indicate  that  fighting  is  a  poor  measure  of 
social  rank  and  is  related  only  indirectly  to  it.  These  results  appear  to 
contradict  the  results  of  other  experiments,  although  these  differences 
would  probably  be  reconcilable  if  more  detailed  information  were  available 
on  social  behavior  in  these  animals.  Rather  rigid  and  simple  criteria  are 
used  to  determine  rank  in  animals,  such  as  physical  dominance-subordi- 
nance relationships,  and  it  is  quite  clear  that  social  interactions  are  far 
more  complex  than  this.  However,  the  apparently  contradictory  results 
with  relation  to  fighting  and  dominance  in  no  way  invalidate  the  conclu- 
sions that  purely  psychological  social  pressures  are  responsible  for  stimu- 
lating increased  adrenocortical  and  decreased  reproductive  activity  in 
groups  of  mice  and  rats  and  that  fighting  per  se  has  little  or  no  effect  on 
the  adrenal  hypertrophy  observed  in  groups  of  animals.  One  can  speculate 
with  some  reason  that  a  massive  stimulation  of  the  nervous  system  and 
adrenal  medulla  occurs  in  subordinate  animals  when  they  are  suddenly 
confronted  with  a  dominant,  aggressive  male.  Observation  shows  that 
these  animals  are  aware  of  their  rank  and  cower  in  front  of  the  dominant 
animal.  When  there  is  no  escape  from  constant  contact,  it  seems  inevitable 
that  the  subordinate  animals  must  suffer  from  emotional  anxiety  resulting 
from  a  desire  to  escape  from  the  situation  and  the  inability  to  do  so.  Chronic 
stimulation  of  the  pituitary-adrenocortical  system  presumably  results  from 
the  chronic  continuation  of  such  a  situation.  Admittedly  this  somewhat 
anthropomorphic  interpretation  of  the  situation  is  largely  speculative,  but 
observations  of  mice  in  groups  inevitably  lead  one  to  such  a  conclusion. 

Finally,  Mason  (1959)  has  shown  that  when  rhesus  monkeys  are  kept  in 


268  /.  /•  Christian 

groups  for  prolonged  periods  of  time  there  is  a  significant  increase  in  their 
urinary  tetrahydroxycorticoids,  the  metabolites  of  the  carbohydrate-active 
corticoids,  excreted  by  grouped  monkeys  is  greater  than  their  combined 
daily  production  of  corticoids  when  individually  caged.  Furthermore,  the 
production  of  corticoids  remains  high  for  the  entire  period  of  grouping.  It 
has  also  been  observed  that  when  human  bomber  crews  are  housed  as  a 
group,  their  production  of  urinary  corticoids  is  increased  over  the  combined 
individual  production  of  corticoids  by  the  same  men,  paralleling  the  findings 
for  monkeys  (Mason,  1959) .  In  the  case  of  monkeys  and  men  there  can  be 
no  question  that  the  stimulus  to  increased  corticosteroid  production  is 
psychological,  resulting  from  social  interactions. 

There  can  be  little  doubt  that  social  pressures  can  increase  pituitary- 
adrenocortical  activity.  We  have  also  suggested  that  there  is  a  depression  of 
reproductive  function  in  male  mice,  as  indicated  by  the  weights  of  the  sex 
accessories,  coinciding  with  increased  adrenocortical  function  in  relation  to 
social  factors.  These  results  are  in  agreement  with  the  earlier  work  of  Crew 
and  Mirskaia  ( 1931 )  and  Retzlaff  ( 1938) ,  who  showed  that  increased  popu- 
lation density  depresses  reproduction  in  female  mice.  The  reproductive 
performance  of  female  mice  was  inversely  related  to  population  sizes  in 
populations  of  1,  4,  8,  or  12  pairs.  Retzlaff  (1938)  also  indicated  that  repro- 
ductive performance  was  best  in  the  socially  dominant  females  in  each 
population.  He  made  several  additional  observations  of  interest  in  these 
experiments.  He  noted  that  there  were  aggressively  dominant  females  that 
attacked  and  viciously  fought  introduced  females  or  females  which  had 
been  removed  and  were  later  replaced.  The  subordinate  females  suffered 
death  or  injury,  and,  of  particular  interest,  any  infections  that  they  had 
were  greatly  exaggerated.  This  is  one  of  the  early  experunental  indications 
of  decreased  resistance  to  disease  following  social  stress,  and  is  further 
evidence  that  resistance  is  decreased  primarily  in  the  subordinate  animals. 
He  also  noted  that  in  the  largest  populations,  12  pairs  of  mice,  there  was  a 
sufficient  confusion  among  the  mice  to  offer  the  subordinate  animals  partial 
protection  from  attack.  Similar  effects  were  later  noted  in  populations  of 
32  male  mice  (Christian,  1955a,  b).  Finally,  it  was  found  that  a  reduction 
in  environmental  temperature  of  approximately  16°  C.  resulted  in  a  signifi- 
cant decline  in  mean  litter  size  for  mice  from  populations  of  1,  2,  or  8  pairs, 
but  not  from  populations  of  4  or  12.  The  protection  against  a  reduction  in 
litter  size  by  decreased  temperature  probably  reflects  huddling  as  a  means 
of  maintaining  body  heat  and  therefore  diminishing  the  need  for  increasing 
thyroid  activity.  Retzlaff  could  not  explain  the  reduction  in  populations  of 
8,  but  evidently  it  was  due  to  severe  social  strife  rather  than  the  reduction 
in  temperature.  It  is  conceivable  that  severe  strife  prevented  huddling  due 
to  mutual  intolerance;  so  that  both  factors  could  play  a  causative  role  in 


2.  Endocrines  and  Populations  269 

reducing  reproductive  performance.  The  preceding  experiments  usually- 
used  populations  consisting  entirely  of  male  or  mixed  male  and  female  mice 
or  rats  (Crew  and  Mirskaia,  1931 ;  Retzlaff,  1938;  Barnett,  1958) .  However, 
a  depression  of  reproductive  function  occurs  when  only  female  mice  are 
placed  in  groups  (Andervont,  1944).  In  these  latter  experiments  estrous 
cycles  began  at  an  earlier  age,  were  more  frequent,  and  lasted  until  a 
greater  age  in  segregated  female  mice  than  in  their  littermates  kept  in 
groups  of  8  each.  Bullough  (1952)  showed  that  "overcrowding"  mice  (16  to 
a  cage)  for  3  weeks  resulted  in  a  30%  increase  in  the  cross-sectional  area  of 
the  adrenal  cortex  and  an  80%  increase  in  medullary  area.  These  changes 
were  accompanied  by  a  60%  reduction  in  epidermal  mitoses,  which  was 
attributed  to  an  increased  secretion  of  adrenocortical  steroids.  Finally, 
Chitty  (1955)  showed  that  liver  glycogen  was  appreciably  lower  in  voles 
maintained  in  the  laboratory  in  groups  than  in  those  maintained  under 
segregated  conditions. 

2.  Adrenocortical  and  Reproductive  Responses  to  Population 
Density 

The  preceding  experiments  indicate  that  sociopsychologic  factors  in- 
volved in  social  interactions  between  mammals  can  elicit  physiologic 
adaptive  responses  with  increased  pituitary-adrenocortical  and  decreased 
reproductive  function  in  voles,  house  mice,  both  wild  and  albino,  Norway 
rats,  rhesus  monkeys,  and  humans.  However,  with  the  exception  of  the 
experiments  of  Crew  and  Mirskaia  (1931)  and  Retzlaff  (1938)  there  was 
no  indication  that  these  functions  were  altered  in  relation  to  population 
density.  Although  an  inverse  relation  between  population  size  and  reproduc- 
tive performance  was  shown  in  the  experiments  of  Crew  and  Mirskaia 
(1931)  and  Retzlaff  (1938),  these  authors  did  not  investigate  adrenocorti- 
cal function.  If  social  competition,  as  a  stimulus  to  increased  pituitary- 
adrenocortical  and  decreased  reproductive  activity,  is  responsible  for  regu- 
lating the  growth  and  decline  of  mammalian  populations,  then  there  must 
be  a  relationship  between  population  density  and  the  magnitude  of  the 
endocrine  responses.  The  existence  of  such  a  relationship  has  been  demon- 
strated in  a  variety  of  experiments. 

If  male  mice  which  have  been  segregated  since  weaning  are  placed  to- 
gether in  groups  of  1,  4,  8,  16,  or  32  per  cage  for  a  week,  there  is  a  hyper- 
trophy of  the  adrenal  glands  and  atrophy  of  the  gonads  and  sex  accessories 
which  progresses  more  or  less  linearly  as  the  logarithm  of  the  population 
increases  (Christian,  1959b) .  There  is  a  decline  in  thymus  weight  from  that 
of  the  isolated  controls,  but  the  decrease  is  not  related  to  the  population 
size.  The  increase  in  adrenal  weight  was  found  to  result  primarily  from 


270  /•  J-  Christian 

cellular  hyperplasia  and  hypertrophy  of  the  zona  f asciculata,  although  the 
glands  were  not  critically  examined  for  medullary  hypertrophy.  The  decline 
in  the  weights  of  the  testes  was  a  reflection  of  the  generalized  reduction  in 
body  weight  in  one  series  of  experiments  (Christian,  1955a),  but  in  another 
series  the  testes  declined  in  relative  testicular  weight  as  well  as  in  absolute 
weight  with  increasing  population  size  (Christian,  1955b).  Body  weight 
was  significantly  less  in  populations  of  4,  8,  and  16. 

These  experiments  were  repeated  using  wild  house  mice  raised  in  the 
laboratory  in  populations  of  1,  3,  4,  6,  8,  9,  and  17  with  similar  though 
more  pronounced  results  (Christian,  1955b).  The  thymus  weight  of  these 
mice  decreased  markedly  as  population  size  increased.  The  adrenocortical 
response  was  also  much  more  pronounced  in  these  mice  than  in  the  albino 
mice,  although  the  mean  weights  of  the  adrenals  of  segregated  albino  and 
brown  male  mice  were  identical.  The  increase  in  adrenal  weight  of  albino 
mice  reached  a  maximum  of  8%  above  the  control  levels  in  populations  of 
16,  whereas  that  of  the  wild  stock  attained  a  maximum  increase  of  21% 
above  the  control  levels  in  populations  of  9.  In  populations  of  32  albino 
mice  and  17  wild  mice  the  mean  adrenal  weight  was  less  than  in  the  next 
smaller  population  size  in  each  case.  This  was  found  to  be  due  to  a  loss  of 
lipid  and  a  marked  decrease  in  the  size  of  cells  in  the  zona  fasciculata  al- 
though the  degree  of  hyperplasia  was  greater  than  in  the  adrenals  of  mice 
from  the  preceding  population  size  (Christian,  1959b).  The  wild  mice  are 
much  more  alert,  reactive,  and  aggressive  than  albino  mice,  and  the 
differences  in  adrenal  reactivity  probably  reflect  such  behavioral  differences. 

The  decline  in  the  weights  of  the  accessory  reproductive  organs,  seminal 
vesicles,  and  preputial  glands  indicated  a  diminished  secretion  of  androgens 
from  the  testes  with  increasing  population  size,  based  on  the  assumption 
that  these  organs  accurately  reflect  androgen  levels  (Burrows,  1949;  Ren- 
nels  ct  al,  1953) .  This  conclusion  is  strengthened  by  the  fact  that  relative 
testicular  weight  also  declined  in  one  group  of  experiments  (Christian, 
1955b)  and  absolute  weight  declined  in  all  experiments  with  increased 
population  size.  Since  all  indications  point  to  a  decline  in  the  secretion  of 
androgens,  the  decline  in  thymus  weight  must  therefore  represent  involu- 
tion by  increased  amounts  of  circulating  corticoids. 

The  progressive  hypertrophy  of  the  adrenals  and  atrophy  of  the  thymus 
and  reproductive  organs  do  not  reflect  a  diminished  space  per  mouse  per 
se,  as  the  results  were  essentially  identical  where  populations  of  1,  4,  6,  8, 
and  17  each  of  male  wild  mice  were  placed  in  cages  with  42  times  the  area 
of  the  cages  used  in  the  preceding  experiments  (Christian,  1959b).  The 
similarity  of  the  results  from  these  two  series  of  experiments  with  vastly 
different  amounts  of  area  per  mouse  also  suggests  that  the  amount  of 
exercise  or  activity  was  not  a  factor  in  the  observed  endocrine  responses. 


2.  Endocrines  and  Populations  271 

It  has  been  established  that  close  confinement  with  adequate  feeding  can 
result  in  adrenocortical  atrophy  and  lymphoid  hypertrophy  in  a  variety  of 
species  of  mammals  (Christian  and  Ratcliffe,  1952),  but  these  limits  of 
confinement  evidently  were  not  approached  in  the  above  experiments,  as 
indicated  by  the  adrenal  weights  in  the  large  and  small  cages. 

These  results  apply  to  male  mice.  When  female  mice  are  grouped,  there 
is  evidence  of  increased  adrenocortical  activity,  increased  corticoids  and 
androgens  being  secreted  (Christian,  1960).  The  degree  of  hypertrophy  is 
small,  however  being  nowhere  nearly  the  amount  seen  in  grouped  male  mice 
or  even  in  females  from  populations  of  mixed  sex.  The  inference  is  that  the 
cortical  hypertrophy  seen  in  females  from  populations  of  mixed  sex  must  to 
a  large  degree  reflect  a  situation  created  primarily  by  the  males. 

3.  Food  and  Social  Competition;  Splenic  Hypertrophy 

In  all  the  experiments  with  mice  so  far  discussed,  food  and  water  were 
provided  ad  libitum.  Food  was  scattered  over  the  cage  and  water  was 
available  from  several  sources  in  order  to  avoid  competition  for  food  which 
might  result  in  inanition  in  the  subordinate  animals  and  might  constitute 
stimuli  to  the  endocrine  adaptive  responses.  These  precautions  were  taken 
even  though  observation  had  indicated  that  feeding  and  drinking  were 
more  or  less  random  and  on  an  individual  basis,  and  that  there  was  no 
observable  competition  for  these  items.  Also  Uhrich  (1938)  and  Strecker 
and  Emlen  (1953)  had  indicated  that  a  limited  supply  of  food  did  not  in- 
crease competition  among  house  mice.  Nevertheless,  the  question  whether 
or  not  the  location  and  amount  of  food  was  a  major  factor  in  eliciting  the 
observed  changes  in  grouped  mice  in  the  pituitary-adrenal  and  pituitary- 
gonadal  systems  was  answered  by  specifically  designed  experiments.  In 
one  series  of  experiments,  male  mice,  some  in  groups  of  four  each  and  some 
segregated,  were  provided  food  either  ad  libitum  or  limited  to  4.0  gm.  per 
mouse  per  day  for  a  7-day  experimental  period.  This  amount  of  food  re- 
stricted weight  gain  but  did  not  produce  weight  loss.  The  food  given  the 
grouped  animals,  whether  limited  or  ad  libitum,  was  scattered  for  half  of 
the  populations  and  supplied  from  a  feeder  for  the  other  half.  Therefore 
food  was  given  the  mice  in  one  of  the  four  following  ways:  scattered  and 
limited  in  amounts,  from  a  feeder  and  limited,  scattered  and  ad  libitum,  or 
from  a  feeder  ad  libitum.  The  experiment  was  repeated  using  groups  of 
five  each  and  a  food  limitation  of  3.5  gm.  per  mouse  per  day,  an  amount 
which  produced  an  appreciable  weight  loss  in  all  the  mice  during  the  week 
of  the  experiment.  This  amount  of  food  per  mouse  per  day  can  therefore  be 
said  to  produce  inanition.  It  was  found  that  neither  the  location  of  food, 
whether  scattered  or  from  a  feeder,  nor  the  amount  of  food  had  any  effect 


272  J.  J.  Christian 

on  adrenal  weight  in  the  segregated  or  grouped  mice.  (Irouping  produced 
significant  adrenal  and  splenic  hypertrophy  along  with  atrophy  of  the 
thymus  and  reproductive  organs,  and  these  changes  were  not  altered  by 
the  source  or  amount  of  food.  The  splenic  hypertrophy  produced  in  the 
albino  house  mice  in  these  experiments  by  grouping  paralleled  the  splenic 
hypertrophy  in  voles  following  social  "stress"  (Clarke,  1953;  Chitty  et  at., 
1956).  However,  restricting  the  amount  of  food  resulted  in  an  atrophy  of 
the  reproductive  organs,  thymus,  and  spleen  which  was  related  to  the 
degree  of  food  restriction.  The  source  of  food  was  without  effect  on  the 
adrenals,  thymus,  spleen,  or  reproductive  organs.  Therefore  the  source  or 
amount  of  food  did  not  increase  the  level  of  social  competition  above  that 
already  present  as  a  result  of  the  establishment  of  a  social  order.  The 
amount  of  food  provided  had  no  effect  on  the  adrenal  glands,  therefore  it 
was  concluded  from  these  and  the  experiments  of  others  (Mulinos  and 
Pomerantz,  1941;  D'Angelo  ct  al.,  1948;  Baker,  1952)  that  inanition  does 
not  constitute  a  stimulus  to  the  pituitary-adrenocortical  system  of  house 
mice  and  rats. 

On  the  other  hand,  Frank's  (1953)  experiments  with  confined  popula- 
tions of  meadow  voles  (Microtus  arvalis)  suggest  that  a  deficient  supply  of 
food  may  increase  social  competition  in  this  species,  as  a  marked  increase 
in  fighting  occurred  following  the  development  of  a  food  shortage.  Whether 
or  not  a  food  shortage  will  increase  competition  very  likely  depends  on  the 
time  relationships  of  the  feeding  behavior  of  the  species.  One  would  not 
expect  to  find  increased  competition  among  animals  which  feed  randomly 
with  respect  to  time,  otherwise  one  would  have  to  attribute  to  these  animals 
the  ability  to  predict,  ahead  of  time,  the  development  of  a  shortage,  as 
there  would  be  no  appreciable  increase  in  competition  for  food  at  any  given 
moment.  This  consideration  also  implies  that  dominant  and  subordinate 
animals  would  lose  weight  equally  in  the  presence  of  a  deficient  food  supply. 
Such  was  actually  the  case  in  the  experiments  with  house  mice  (Christian, 
1959c) .  On  the  other  hand,  an  increase  in  competition  would  be  expected  to 
follow  the  development  of  a  food  shortage  among  mammals  which  habi- 
tually feed  during  the  same  period  of  time  every  day.  In  this  situation  one 
would  not  expect  the  dominant  animals  to  lose  weight  to  the  same  degree 
as  the  subordinate  animals,  if  at  all.  Frank's  (1953)  results  coincide  with 
this  latter  situation.  If  these  conjectures  are  correct,  it  is  apparent  that  the 
temporal  feeding  relationship  and  behavior  of  any  species  will  assume  para- 
mount importance  with  respect  to  the  production  of  competition  and  the 
physiologic  responses  to  it,  and  should  therefore  be  investigated  critically 
for  a  variety  of  mammals.  The  experiments  with  house  mice  should  be  re- 
peated with  voles  and  other  species  and  any  increases  in  competition  deter- 
mined by  observation  and  the  accompanying  adrenocortical  responses. 


2.  Endocrines  and  Populations  273 

Although  inanition  and  starvation  may  not  be  stimuli  to  increased 
pituitary-adrenocortical  activity  in  mice  and  rats,  they  may  be  in  guinea 
pigs  (D'Angelo  et  al.,  1948)  and  white-footed  mice  {Pero7nyscus  leucopus) 
(Sealander,  1950).  However,  a  limited  amount  of  food  does  depress  repro- 
ductive activity  in  house  mice  as  well  as  in  other  species  which  have  been 
investigated  (Lutwak-Mann,  1958),  possibly  as  a  resvilt  of  a  protein  de- 
ficiency, as  it  has  been  shown  that  a  dietary  deficiency  of  protein  diminishes 
the  secretion  of  gonadotropins  (Srebnik  et  al.,  1958;  Lutwak-Mann,  1958). 
The  striking  declines  in  the  weights  of  the  preputial  glands,  and  especially 
of  the  seminal  vesicles,  brought  about  by  a  limited  amount  of  food  (Chris- 
tian, 1959c)  indicate  that  the  secretion  of  androgens  by  the  testes  was 
markedly  depressed.  Whatever  the  mechanism  by  which  a  food  deficiency 
depresses  the  secretion  of  gonadotropins,  it  was  not  by  eliciting  a  generalized 
adaptive  response  involving  the  pituitary-adrenocortical  system.  These 
results  lead  to  several  conclusions,  at  least  as  far  as  mice  are  concerned. 
One  is  that  when  food  is  supplied  ad  libitum,  competition  for  food  is  not  a 
factor  in  eliciting  pituitary-adrenocortical  responses  in  relation  to  popula- 
tion. A  second  is  that  inanition  and  starvation  per  se  are  not  stimuli  to 
increased  pituitary  adrenocortical  activity  in  mice  or  rats.  The  third  is 
that  food  restriction  depresses  reproduction  by  dimmishing  the  secretion 
of  gonadotropins  without  eliciting  a  more  widespread  response,  and  there- 
fore a  limited  supply  of  food  can  limit  population  growth  specifically  by 
depressing  reproduction  without  operating  through  the  pituitary-adreno- 
cortical system.  However,  we  have  seen  that  there  is  some  indication  that 
these  relationships  may  not  be  universally  true.  Perhaps  food  supplies  and 
social  competition  can  act  independently  to  limit  population  growth.  Food 
conceivably  may  not  be  important  to  natural  populations  as  long  as  sub- 
ordmate  animals  are  free  to  move  elsewhere.  Calhoun  (1949,  1950)  has 
indicated  that  social  competition  is  a  more  important  factor  than  food 
supply  per  se  for  Norway  rats.  One  fact  is  abundantly  clear  as  a  result  of 
these  experiments,  and  that  is  that  sociopsychologic  factors  stimulate  in- 
creased pituitary-adrenocortical  function  and  depress  reproductive  func- 
tion in  proportion  to  population  density ;  this  reaction  system  is  therefore 
active  at  all  levels  of  population  in  the  control  of  population  growth, 
whereas  a  limitation  in  the  food  supply  will  exert  its  effects  on  reproduction 
only  when  it  results  in  inanition  in  the  members  of  a  population.  These 
considerations  are  of  obvious  importance  to  the  investigator  interested  in 
the  control  and  regulation  of  mammalian  populations. 

The  production  of  splenic  hypertrophy  in  highly  inbred  albino  mice  by 
increased  population  density  is  especially  interesting  in  view  of  Chitty's 
(1957)  conclusion  that  the  splenic  hypertrophy  in  voles  {Microtus  agrestis 
and  M .  orcadensis)  was  due  to  inherited  genetic  factors.  This  conclusion 


274  J.   J.   Christian 

was  based  on  the  fact  that  splenic  hypertrophy,  which  had  been  observed 
previously  only  in  natural  populations  of  high  density,  had  made  its  ap- 
pearance recently  in  laboratory  stocks  of  these  voles.  The  only  factor  which 
seemed  to  account  for  this  appearance  was  the  introduction  into  the  labora- 
tory breeding  population  of  voles  from  natural  populations  exhibiting 
splenic  enlargement.  The  fact  that  a  similar  hypertrophj'  was  observed  in 
response  to  increased  population  density  in  a  highly  inbred  strain  of  mice, 
maintained  under  constant  conditions  for  a  good  many  years  with  no  expo- 
sure to  natiu-al  conditions,  makes  a  genetic  explanation  of  the  sort  postu- 
lated by  D.  Chitty  unlikely  in  such  an  inbred  strain  of  presumptively  gene- 
tically stable  mice.  Similarly,  as  was  pointed  out  earlier,  a  comparable 
hypertrophy  of  the  spleen  was  observed  in  a  few  inbred  rats  subjected  to 
alarming  stimuli.  The  hypertrophy,  when  examined  critically,  has  been 
found  to  be  due  to  increased  hematopoiesis  (Dawson,  1956),  possibly  in 
response  to  the  increased  stimulation  of  erythropoiesis  by  hormonal  factors. 
It  is  also  well  known  that  splenic  erythropoiesis  can  be  stimulated  by  any 
stimulus  that  produces  anemia.  Therefore,  conclusions  regarding  the  causes 
of  splenic  hypertrophy,  due  to  increased  erythropoiesis,  must  be  inter- 
preted with  caution;  although  it  does  not  seem  likely  that  it  is  genetic  in 
origin,  at  least  in  inbred  albino  mice  with  splenic  hypertrophy  following 
increased  population  density. 

4.  Reproductive  Fuxctiox  in  Female  Mice:  Lactation,  Reproduc- 
tion 

Female  mammals  have  frequently  been  observed  to  respond  to  adverse 
stimuli  with  a  reduction  of  reproductive  function.  When  rats  are  suddenly 
moved  from  a  temperature  to  which  they  have  become  accustomed  to  a 
different  temperature,  either  higher  or  lower,  there  is  retardation  of  growth 
and  prolongation  of  the  estrous  cycle  (Bohanan,  1939).  Selye  (1939)  has 
listed  a  variety  of  agents  which  will  inhibit  o^'arian  function  and  estrus. 
Reproduction  is  also  depressed  in  female  mice  by  increased  population 
density.  We  have  noted  that  Crew  and  Mirskaia  (1931)  and  Retzlaff  (1938) 
found  that  reproductive  performance  of  female  albino  mice  declined  with 
increasing  population  density.  In  another  series  of  experiments  no  young 
were  produced  and  no  females  became  visibly  pregnant  when  mice  were 
crowded  20  males  and  20  females  to  a  cage  for  6  weeks  (Christian  and  Le- 
Munyan,  1958) .  It  is  not  known  whether  there  was  a  marked  suppression 
of  ovarian  function  with  diminished  ovulation,  a  failure  of  the  ova  to  im- 
plant, or  intra-uterine  loss  early  in  pregnancy,  but  more  than  likely  all 
these  factors  were  involved.  When  the  population  size  was  reduced  to  10 
males  and  10  females,  all  the  females  became  pregnant,  but  the  number  of 


2.  Endocrines  and  Populations  275 

implanted  ova  was  reduced  significantly  and  only  7  of  the  10  females 
delivered  young.  The  remainder  lost  their  progeny  in  utero  during  the  early 
stages  of  pregnancy.  The  onset  of  pregnancy  also  was  considerably  delayed 
in  these  animals.  Therefore  there  was  decreased  fertility,  decreased  im- 
plantation, and  a  marked  increase  in  intra-uterine  mortality.  These  results 
indicate  that  female  mice  respond  to  increased  population  density  with  a 
depression  of  reproductive  function  at  all  stages  of  the  processes.  These 
results  correspond  to  those  seen  in  males  as  indicated  by  the  weights  of 
their  reproductive  organs.  Reproductive  suppression  was  also  observed 
when  female  mice  were  grouped  without  males  (Andervont,  1944;  Whitten, 
1959;  Christian,  1960). 

Chitty  (1952)  noticed  that  young  voles  from  natural  populations  of  high 
density  were  reduced  in  size  and  were  unusually  susceptible  to  increased 
mortality.  He  hypothesized  that  the  young  were  adversely  affected  in  utero 
by  the  physiologic  derangements  in  the  mothers  which  resulted  from  high 
population  densities  (Chitty,  1952,  1954).  Later  it  was  shown  in  a  limited 
series  of  experiments  that  social  "stress"  diminished  lactation  in  voles,  as 
measured  by  the  weights  of  progeny  nursed  by  the  mothers  subjected  to 
social  pressures  which  had  previously  been  shown  to  result  in  increased 
pituitary-adrenocortical  activity  (Chitty,  1955).  These  experiments  were 
repeated  on  a  larger  scale  using  laboratory  white  mice  (Christian  and  Le- 
Munyan,  1958) .  It  was  found  that  progeny  nursed  by  previously  crowded 
mothers  weighed  appreciably  less  at  weaning  than  those  nursed  by  mothers 
which  had  always  been  segregated.  The  effect  on  the  progeny  was  greater 
in  the  larger  litter  sizes,  suggesting  a  quantitative  rather  than  a  qualitative 
deficiency  in  the  supply  of  milk.  When  these  young  which  had  been  nursed 
by  crowded  mothers  were  themselves  bred,  the  progeny  which  they  in  turn 
nursed  were  significantly  lighter  at  weaning  than  their  controls.  Again  the 
defect  was  greatest  in  the  larger  litters,  but  the  difference  was  not  manifest 
until  a  larger  litter  size  had  been  reached  than  in  the  preceding  generation. 
These  differences  in  the  second  filial  generation  cannot  be  attributed  to 
grouping.  Crowding  depresses  all  the  other  reproductive  functions,  therefore 
it  is  not  surprising  that  lactation  is  also  suppressed,  as  it  is  to  a  large  degree 
under  the  control  of  pituitary  gonadotropins  and  sex  steroids  in  addition 
to  oxytocin,  thyroxine,  and  growth  hormone  (Folley,  1956;  Grosvenor  and 
Turner,  1959a,  b,  c,).  These  results  are  consistent  with  others  which  show 
that  lactation  can  be  inhibited  by  a  variety  of  stimuli  which  stimulate  in- 
creased pituitary-adrenocortical  activity  and  diminish  increased  reproduc- 
tion and  growth  (Selye,  1954) ;  in  fact,  lactation  can  be  limited  by  a  defi- 
ciency of  any  one  of  a  variety  of  hormones  necessary  for  its  fulfillment 
(Grosvenor  and  Turner,  1959a,  b,  c).  Since  the  young  mice  in  litters  of 
small  size  were  unaffected  in  the  above  experiments,  it  is  unlikely  that  any 


276  /.  /.   Christian 

substance,  such  as  adrenal  hormones,  contained  in  the  milk  were  responsible 
for  the  decrease  in  weights  of  the  progeny.  The  mechanism  by  which  these 
effects  were  carried  over  into  the  second  generation  of  progeny  is  not  known. 
Presumably  the  second  generation  of  young  may  reflect  the  inanition  suf- 
fered by  the  first  generation  of  progeny  as  a  result  of  deficient  lactation. 
However,  the  fact  that  androgens  can  produce  subsequent  permanent 
sterility  when  injected  into  mice  less  than  10  days  old  suggests  the  possibil- 
ity that  increased  amounts  of  adrenal  androgens  may  reach  the  nursing 
young  via  the  milk  and  exert  similar  partial  effects  if  some  escape  metabo- 
lism in  the  liver.  The  weights  of  the  young  at  birth  were  unaffected  by  the 
earlier  crowding  of  their  mothers.  Although  the  precise  mechanisms  are  not 
understood,  these  results  support  and  extend  D.  Chitty's  results  with  voles 
and  help  in  providing  an  explanation  for  the  prolonged  effects  of  high 
density  on  surviving  young  observed  in  natural  populations,  such  as 
Chitty's  (1952,  1954)  observations  on  young  voles  from  natural  popula- 
tions. 

5.  Growth 

The  effects  of  increased  population  density  on  growth  have  not  been 
studied  to  the  same  degree  as  its  effects  on  other  aspects  of  endocrine 
physiology,  although  suppression  of  the  secretion  of  pituitary  growth 
hormone  presumably  is  a  part  of  the  response  to  stimuli  which  also  result 
in  the  increased  secretion  of  ACTH  (Selye,  1950) .  However,  there  are  a  few 
experiments  which  clearlj^  indicate  that  the  growth  of  house  mice  is  de- 
pressed in  response  to  increased  population  density  (Vetulani,  1931; 
Christian,  1955b) .  It  has  been  shown  also  that  there  is  suppression  of 
growth  in  all  but  the  dominant  and  second-ranking  mice  in  a  group  of  six 
(Christian,  1961).  The  degree  of  inhibition  was  related  to  rank. 

6.  Inflammation,  Resistance  to  Infection,  and  Population  Den- 
sity 

In  earlier  sections  of  this  account  the  inhibition  of  the  inflammatory  re- 
response,  granulation,  and  antibody  formation,  and  therefore  resistance  to 
disease,  by  adrenal  carbohydrate-active  corticoids  was  discussed.  Most  of 
the  experimental  evidence  cited  was  based  on  the  results  of  injecting  corti- 
cal hormones.  A  few  examples  of  decreased  resistance  to  infectious  agents 
brought  about  by  stimuli,  such  as  cold,  which  stimulated  increased  adreno- 
cortical secretion  were  given.  However,  even  though  these  experiments 
indicated  that  such  effects  might  occm-  under  natural  circumstances,  they 
did  not  establish  this  possibility,  and  especially  they  did  not  show  that 
increased  population  density  could  stimulate  a  sufficient  increase  in  the 


2.  Endocrines  and  Populations  277 

secretion  of  adrenal  carbohydrate-active  corticoids  to  inhibit  inflammation, 
granulation,  and  antibody  formation  sufficiently  to  decrease  resistance  to 
disease,  although  Retzlaff  (1938)  had  indicated  that  there  was  decreased 
resistance  to  infection  in  the  subordinate  mice  in  his  population  studies. 
Experiments  have  been  conducted  which  do  establish  these  points  to  a 
limited  degree,  but  more  experiments  with  more  species  are  needed. 

An  efficient  method  of  inducing  an  inflammatory  response  and  the  forma- 
tion of  granulation  tissue  in  rats  or  mice  is  to  implant  subcutaneously  cotton 
pellets  moistened  with  turpentine  (Meier  et  at.,  1950;  Christian  and  Wil- 
liamson, 1958).  Later  these  pellets  and  the  surrounding  tissue  can  be  re- 
moved and  weighed.  In  this  fashion  the  degree  of  the  inflammatory  response 
and  formation  of  granulation  tissue  can  be  assayed  under  a  variety  of 
conditions.  This  technique  has  been  used  to  show  that  ACTH  and  the 
adrenal  carbohydrate-active  corticoids  suppress  these  responses  (Meier 
ct  al.,  1950) .  By  this  procedure  it  was  found  that  when  mice  w^ere  placed 
in  groups  of  5  each  there  was  approximately  a  20%  reduction  in  the  amount 
of  granulation  tissue  formed  in  1  week  compared  to  the  amount  formed  by 
the  segregated  control  mice  (Christian  and  Williamson,  1958).  These 
experiments  clearly  indicated  that  increased  population  densit}^  is  a  suffi- 
cient stimulus  to  the  pituitary-adrenocortical  system  to  decrease  signifi- 
cantly inflammation  and  granulation. 

Davis  and  Read  (1958)  conducted  a  series  of  related  experiments  in 
which  they  demonstrated  that  placing  wild-stock  house  mice  in  groups 
markedly  increased  the  susceptibility  of  the  mice  to  invasion  by  the  larvae 
of  TrichincUa  spiralis.  Each  mouse  was  infected  parenterally  with  approxi- 
mately 125  embryonated  Trichinella  larvae.  Each  mouse  was  maintained  in 
a  separate  cage,  but  from  day  3  through  11  after  infection  11  of  the  mice 
were  placed  in  two  groups,  one  of  5  and  the  other  of  6  mice,  for  3  hours  a 
day,  while  11  others  were  left  segregated.  The  mice  were  sacrificed  the  15th 
day  after  infection,  the  gastrointestinal  tracts  were  digested,  and  the  larval 
worms  were  recovered.  Only  3  of  the  segregated  mice  were  infected  with  an 
average  of  9  worms  apiece,  whereas  all  the  grouped  mice  were  infected  with 
an  average  of  32  worms  each,  a  250%  increase.  The  experiment  was  re- 
peated with  6  segregated  and  5  grouped  mice,  but  they  were  not  sacrificed 
until  the  30th  day  post  infection,  and  the  encysted  larvae  were  recovered. 
All  the  mice,  segregated  and  grouped,  were  infected,  but  the  grouped  mice 
had  48%  more  encysted  larvae  than  their  segregated  controls.  In  both 
experiments  the  differences  in  the  number  of  worms  between  segregated 
and  grouped  mice  were  highly  significant  and  in  both  experiments  the  mean 
adrenal  weight  was  greater  in  the  grouped  than  in  the  segregated  mice,  8% 
iP  <  0.20)  in  the  first  experiment  and  20%  (P  <  0.01)  in  the  second 
experiment. 


278  /.  /,   Christian 

In  earlier  experiments  it  had  been  shown  that  either  cortisone  or  ACTH 
increased  the  invasiveness  of  Trichinella  larvae  by  suppressing  the  defen- 
sive inflammatory  response  of  the  host's  intestinal  wall  and  possibly  by 
prolonging  the  sojourn  of  the  adult  females  in  the  gut  by  suppressmg  im- 
mune responses  to  the  worms  (Stoner  and  Godwin,  1953).  Thus,  these 
hormones  decreased  the  resistance  of  mice  to  invasion  by  the  larvae  of 
Trichinella  spiralis  by  inhibiting  inflammation  and  possibly  antibody 
formation.  Grouping  evidently  stimulated  a  sufficient  increase  in  the  secre- 
tion of  adrenal  corticoids  to  produce  similar  effects.  This  conclusion  is  sup- 
ported by  the  demonstration  that  grouping  was  sufficient  stimulus  to 
pituitary-adrenocortical  activity  to  diminish  granuloma  formation  appreci- 
ably. These  experiments  with  trichinosis  in  house  mice  have  been  repeated 
with  albino  mice  with  similar  results,  confirming  the  original  results  and 
demonstrating  that  albino  and  wild-strain  house  mice  react  similarly  (Davis 
and  Read,  unpublished)  .- 

The  effects  of  crowding  on  trichinosis  in  mice  is  presumably  primarily  due 
to  the  effects  of  increased  adrenocortical  secretion  on  the  inflammatory 
response  to  the  worms.  Therefore  a  similar  series  of  experiments  were  per- 
formed which  were  more  specificaUy  designed  to  demonstrate  the  effects  of 
grouping  on  antibody  formation  (Davis  and  Prudovsky,  1959).  In  this  case 
mice  were  injected  with  tetanus  toxoid  and  challenged  10  days  later  with 
13  mouse  MLD  of  tetanus  toxin.  The  dosage  of  toxin  was  selected  to  cause 
death  in  50%  of  segregated  mice.  Mice  were  placed  in  groups  either  5  or  3 
days  prior  to  giving  the  toxoid  and  left  in  groups  until  5  days  after  the 
toxoid.  Control  mice  were  left  segregated  in  individual  cages  throughout 
the  experimental  period  but  otherwise  were  treated  identically  to  the  experi- 
mental animals.  Grouping  appreciably  decreased  resistance  and  increased 
mortality  of  mice  to  the  challenge  dose  over  the  control  levels.  This  effect 
was  less  marked  when  the  mice  were  grouped  3  days  before  administering 
the  toxoid.  The  effects  of  grouping  resembled  the  effects  of  injected  corti- 
sone at  the  appropriate  times  and  in  appropriate  doses.  These  results  indi- 

^  A  report  was  published  recently  which  implied  that  cortisone  or  ACTH  are  without 
effect  on  the  course  of  trichinosis  in  mice,  but  which  showed  that  cortisone  markedl}' 
increased  mortality  (Lord,  1958).  These  experiments  require  some  critical  comment. 
The  procedure  used  to  infect  the  mice  was  similar  to  that  used  by  Davis  and  Read 
(1958),  but  injections  of  cortisone  and  ACTH  were  not  begun  until  6  days  after  infection. 
One  would  not  anticipate  that  these  hormones  would  affect  the  course  of  trichinosis  at 
that  late  date,  as  invasion  by  the  larval  worms,  the  inflammatorj'  response  to  them  in 
the  intestinal  wall,  and  immune  responses  to  them  are  fairly  well  accomplished  facts 
by  that  time.  Therefore  one  would  not  anticipate  an  alteration  in  the  resistance  to 
invasion  by  the  worms.  The  doses  of  cortisone  used  in  these  experiments  were  extremely 
high  pharmacologic  doses:  76  mg.  in  39  days,  beginning  with  3  mg.  a  day  per  mouse. 
It  is  not  surprising  that  cortisone  increased  mortality. 


2.  Endocrines  and  Populations  279 

cate  that  the  adrenocortical  response  to  grouping  is  sufficient  to  depress 
antibody  formation  as  well  as  inflammation.  From  these  results  one  can 
easily  sec  that  behavioral  factors  associated  with  increases  in  population 
density,  or  grouping,  can  sufficiently  increase  adrenocortical  activity  to 
profoundly  affect  host  resistance  to  infectious  disease  and  parasites. 

The  effects  of  hormones  on  miu-me  tuberculosis  are  somewhat  complex. 
Cortisone  enhances  tuberculosis  infections  m  mice,  especially  by  converthig 
a  smoldering  chronic  infection  into  a  fulminating  acute  process  (Hart  and 
Rees,  1950) .  However,  ACTH  was  without  effect  on  the  early  development 
of  tuberculosis  in  mice  (LeMaistre  et  al.,  1953).  From  these  experiments  it 
would  appear  that  an  increased  secretion  of  endogenous  adrenal  corticoids 
was  ineffective  in  enhancing  tuberculosis  in  mice  whereas  injected  carbohy- 
drate-active corticoids  markedly  enhanced  the  infection.  Therefore,  it  is 
especially  interesting  that  Tobach  and  Block  (1956)  were  able  to  show  that 
crowding  significantly  altered  the  courses  of  acute  and  chronic  tubercu- 
losis m  mice.  Crowding  after  mfection  decreased  the  survival  time  of  mice 
of  both  sexes  suffering  from  an  acute  tuberculous  infection.  Crowding  after 
infection  had  essentially  no  effect  on  the  course  of  chronic  tuberculosis  in 
female  mice,  whereas  it  enhanced  the  chronic  disease  in  male  mice.  These 
experiments  also  indicate  that  crowding  prior  to  infection  may  enhance 
host  resistance. 

These  experiments,  although  quite  limited  in  number,  all  confirm  the 
fact  that  crowding  (increased  population  density)  can  reduce  host  resist- 
ance to  disease.  The  evidence  from  experiments  with  increased  popula- 
tion density  is  completely  in  accord  with  the  results  of  experiments  with 
injected  corticoids.  These  results,  considered  in  the  light  of  the  mass 
of  this  evidence  on  the  effects  of  corticoids  and  ACTH  on  experimental 
infections,  can  only  mean  that  increased  population  density  sufficiently 
stimulates  the  pituitary-adrenocortical  system  to  lower  resistance  to  disease 
by  inhibition  of  inflammation,  granulation,  and  probably  antibody  forma- 
tion by  endogenous  corticoids. 

7.  Production  of  Mortality  Directly 

In  the  preceding  section  the  role  of  behavioral  factors  and  increases  in 
population  density  in  decreasing  resistance  to  disease  was  discussed.  It 
therefore  follows  that  grouping  can  increase  mortality  by  this  means  and 
the  more  subordinate  animals  will  be  the  ones  most  often  affected.  However, 
grouping  is  often  followed  in  a  very  short  tune  by  sudden  death  preceded  by 
alternate  prostration  and  convulsions  of  many  of  the  animals  (Christian, 
1955b),  and  the  greater  the  number  which  are  placed  together,  the  greater 
the  number  which  succumb  in  this  fashion.  (J.  J.  Christian,  unpublished). 


280  J.  J.   Christian 

although  there  is  considerable  variation  from  population  to  population  in 
the  numbers  which  die  in  populations  of  the  same  size.  The  more  reactive 
and  aggressive  wild-stock  house  mice  are  much  more  prone  to  succumb  in 
this  fashion  than  the  usual  albino  mouse  in  the  laboratory  (Christian, 
1955b).  The  proximate  cause  of  sudden  death  in  these  animals  has  not  been 
investigated,  although  its  onset,  behavior,  and  general  symptomatology  arc 
highly  suggestive  of  hypoglycemic  shock  and  closely  resembles  the  deaths 
from  "shock  disease"  observed  in  natural  populations  at  peak  densities 
(Christian,  1950b).  Frank  (1953)  observed  similar  deaths  in  voles  {Micro- 
tus  arvalis)  after  placing  them  in  groups,  as  well  as  in  natural  populations, 
and  was  able  to  demonstrate  that  glucose  could  prevent  the  fatal  termina- 
tion of  the  syndrome,  and  also  showed  that  the  symptomatology  of  insulin 
hypoglycemia  was  identical  to  that  observed  in  voles  dying  naturally 
following  "crowding."  Furthermore,  he  showed  that  the  voles  which  were 
on  the  verge  of  dying  had  markedly  decreased  levels  of  blood  sugar  and 
liver  glycogen.  These  results  make  it  appear  probable  that  the  convulsions 
and  death  which  follow  shortly  on  the  social  strife  produced  by  grouping 
mice  are  due  to  hypoglycemic  shock.  There  is  reason  to  suggest  that  a 
massive  discharge  of  the  adrenal  medulla,  with  its  subsequent  exhaustion 
may  lead  to  a  failure  to  further  mobilize  glucose  and  the  animals  die  in  a 
hypoglycemic  episode.  However,  this  hypothesis  requires  further  testing. 

In  any  event,  social  strife  and  the  physiologic  responses  to  it  may  result 
in  mortality  directly  as  well  as  by  affecting  host  resistance. 

8.  Summary  of  Results  from  Populations  of  Limited  Size 

Experiments  with  populations  of  fixed  size  have  demonstrated  that  socio- 
psychologic  interactions  can  and  do  stimulate  increased  pituitary-adreno- 
cortical  and  decreased  reproductive  activity  and  growth  in  mice,  voles,  and 
Norway  rats.  Increased  adrenocortical  activity  is  related  to  social  domi- 
nance-siibordinance  relationships,  the  more  subordinate  animals  exhibiting 
a  greater  response  than  the  dominant  animals.  These  responses  apparentl}' 
are  unrelated  to  fighting  per  se.  Fighting  is  evidently  a  sign  of  social  interac- 
tion, just  as  is  the  endocrine  response,  and  not  a  causative  factor.  There  is 
a  progressive  increase  in  the  pituitary-adrenocortical  response  and  de- 
crease in  reproductive  function  in  male  and  female  mice  with  increasing 
population  size.  All  phases  of  reproductive  activity  are  depressed,  including 
fertility,  implantation  of  blastulae,  intra-uterine  survival,  estrus,  lactation 
(with  persistent  effects  for  at  least  two  generations) ,  and  the  size  and 
activity  of  the  male  gonads  and  sex  accessories.  Inhibition  of  reproductive 
fimction  apparently  stems  from  a  depressed  secretion  of  pituitary  gonado- 
tropins. Growth  is  also  inhibited  by  increased  population  density. 


2.  Endocrines  and  Populations  281 

In  addition  to  these  primary  effects  of  physiologic  adaptation  to  the 
social  pressures  associated  with  increased  population  density,  there  is  a 
decrease  in  resistance  to  parasitism  and  infection.  It  was  shown  that  in- 
creased density  produced  an  inhibition  of  inflammation,  granulation,  and 
probably  antibody  formation.  A  decrease  in  host  resistance  to  trichinosis 
and  tuberculosis  was  observed  which  apparently  was  due  to  these  factors. 

It  was  also  shown  that  food  was  not  a  factor  in  these  effects,  either  in- 
directly by  increasing  social  competition  or  directly  by  inanition  in  the 
subordinate  animals.  A  deficiency  of  food  is  not  a  stimulus  to  increased 
adrenocortical  activity  in  mice,  or  rats,  although  it  may  be  in  other  species. 
A  food  deficiency,  however,  does  result  in  a  marked  suppression  of  reproduc- 
tive function,  probably  by  decreasing  the  production  of  pituitary  gonado- 
tropins. 

B.  Freely  Growing  Populations 

1.  Introduction 

Experiments  with  populations  of  limited  size,  although  indicative,  do  not 
establish  that  these  same  responses  occur  in  freely  growing  populations.  In 
the  first  place,  experiments  with  limited  populations  were  conducted  for 
limited  periods  of  time,  usually  too  brief  to  permit  adaptation  to  the  situa- 
tion. Furthermore,  in  every  case  the  experimental  approach  involved  sud- 
denly placing  strange  animals  together  under  rather  artificial  circum- 
stances. Therefore,  these  experiments  provided  no  evidence  that  mammals 
which  have  been  more  or  less  in  contact  with  each  other  from  birth  would 
respond  similarly.  House  mice  {Mus  musculus)  and  voles  {Microtus  penn- 
sylvanicus,  Microtus  agrestis)  from  free  growing  populations  show  the  same 
physiologic  responses  to  increased  population  density  as  do  mice  from 
populations  of  fixed  size  but  to  an  even  greater  degree. 

2.  Population  Density  and  Adrenocortical  Function 

Increased  pituitary-adrenocortical  activity  has  been  related  to  increased 
population  density  in  a  number  of  experiments  with  freely  growing  popula- 
tions of  house  mice  and  voles  (Christian,  1956:  Louch,  1956;  Christian, 
1959a,  b).  A  number  of  experimenters  have  shown  that  the  growth  of 
freely  growing  confined  populations  is  self-limited  in  spite  of  the  fact  that 
food,  water,  nesting  material,  and  nesting  space  were  provided  ad  libitum 
and  well  scattered  (Strecker  and  Emlen,  1953;  Clarke,  1955;  Southwick, 
1955a;  Christian,  1956;  Louch,  1956;  Crowcroft  and  Rowe,  1957;  Christian, 
1959b) .  Competition  for  food  or  a  lack  of  availability  of  food  by  particular 
individuals  was  not  a  factor  (Christian,  1956;  Crowcroft  and  Rowe,  1957). 


282  /.  /.  Christian 

The  growth  form  of  these  populations  was  approxunately  sigmoid,  indi- 
cating that  an  intrinsic  damping  factor  was  operating  to  regulate  and  limit 
the  growth  of  populations  throughout  their  histories.  These  populations 
were  started  by  introducing  a  small  number  of  animals  of  both  sexes  into 
confined  quarters  and  allowing  the  population  to  grow  of  its  own  accord. 
The  populations  were  either  sacrificed,  at  maxunal  and  submaximal  levels, 
to  obtain  organ  weights  and  histologic  material,  or  blood  samples  were 
taken  for  eosinophil  counts  to  assess  functional  changes,  especially  of  the 
adrenal  cortex. 

The  zonae  fasciculatae  of  the  adrenal  cortices  were  hyperplastic  and 
hypertrophic  in  house  mice  of  both  sexes  from  populations  of  maximum 
(asymptotic)  size.  The  number  and  size  of  the  cells  of  the  fasciculata  were 
increased.  Adrenal  weight  reflected  the  fascicular  hypertrophy  by  increases 
of  25%  in  the  males  and  14%  in  the  females.  The  adrenal  cortical  hyper- 
trophy was  approximately  half  as  great  in  mice  from  populations  of  approxi- 
mately one  half  the  maximiun  size. 

The  presence  in  mice  of  an  adrenocortical  X-zone  which  is  involuted  by 
androgens  has  been  described.  This  zone  complicates  the  interpretation  of 
adrenal  weight  from  immature  or  puberal  male  or  nulliparous  female  house 
mice  unless  histologic  studies  accompany  the  data  on  weights.  In  the  case 
of  the  freely  growing  populations  of  house  mice  there  was  a  pronounced 
hump  in  the  adrenal  weights  in  relation  to  body  weights  for  mice  from  the 
experimental  populations  in  the  13-19.0-gm.  weight  range,  even  though  the 
fascicular  hypertrophy  was  proportionately  constant  for  all  weights.  The 
curve  of  adrenal  weight  on  body  weight  for  the  segregated  control  mice  was 
more  or  less  regular  with  no  pronounced  irregularities,  although  there  is  a 
tendency  for  the  slope  to  decrease  with  increasing  body  size.  This  appar- 
ently excessive  adrenal  hypertrophy  in  13-19-gm.  mice  from  intermediate 
and  high  populations  resulted  from  a  failure  of  the  X-zone  to  involute 
normally  rather  than  from  a  true  hyperplasia  or  hypertrophy.  The  width  of 
the  X-zone  in  segregated  male  house  mice  begins  to  decline  in  mice  in  the 
10-12.9-gm.  weight  group,  and  involution  is  essentially  complete  by  the 
time  a  body  weight  of  16  gm.  is  reached.  The  decline  did  not  begin  in  the 
experimental  animals  until  a  weight  of  16  gm.  or  more  had  been  reached 
and  was  not  complete  until  a  weight  of  19  gm.  Therefore,  along  with  a 
marked  hyperplasia  of  the  zona  fasciculata,  suggesting  an  increased  secre- 
tion of  corticoids  with  increased  density,  there  was  a  delay  in  the  onset  of 
puberty,  presumably  with  an  insufficient  production  of  androgens  to  in- 
volute the  X-zone  of  male  mice.  A  great  variety  of  steroid  hormones,  in- 
cluding corticoids,  have  been  tested  for  their  ability  to  involute  the  X-zone 
in  male  house  mice  (McPhail  and  Read,  19-42b;  Antopol,  1953;  Allen,  1954; 
Christian,  1954) ,  but  only  those  with  pronounced  androgenic  activity  (e.g., 


2.  Endocrines  and  Populations  283 

testosterone)  have  been  effective,  although  Delost  (1954)  has  reported  that 
cortisone  involutes  the  X-zone  in  voles.  This  delay  in  androgen  production 
presumably  indicated  an  inhibited  secretion  of  gonadotropins  from  the 
anterior  pituitary,  although  there  apparently  were  sufficient  gonadotropins 
to  maintain  the  X-zone,  as  luteinizing  hormone  presumably  is  responsible 
for  maintaining  this  zone  (Jones,  1949b,  1950,  1952). 

Thymic  involution  is  effected  by  carbohydrate-active  corticoids,  estro- 
gens, and  androgens,  with  variations  in  the  mode  of  involution  (cf .  above) . 
Therefore  thymic  involution,  as  a  measure  of  increased  adrenocortical 
secretion,  must  be  interpreted  with  caution  if  there  is  reason  to  suspect 
differences  in  the  levels  of  circulating  sex  steroids.  Such  was  the  case  in 
the  experiments  under  discussion,  but  additional  information  makes  it 
possible  to  state  with  reasonable  certainty  that  the  weights  of  the  thymus 
reflect  increased  corticoid  secretion.  The  thymuses  of  13-19-gm.  male  mice 
from  high  populations  weighed  more  than  their  segregated  controls,  whereas 
the  mean  thymus  weight  of  19-23-gram  mice  from  the  high  populations  was 
less  than  that  of  the  segregated  mice.  The  greater  thymus  weight  coincides 
with  the  greater  width  of  the  adrenal  X-zone  in  the  experimental  mice  and 
probably,  as  in  the  case  of  the  X-zone,  represents  inhibition  of  androgenic 
activity  which  is  not  overridden  by  the  increased  amounts  of  circulating 
corticoids.  It  has  been  shown  that  the  natural  adrenal  secretory  products  of 
mice  will  produce  thymic  involution  and  lymphopenia  (Bahn  ct  al,  1957; 
Wilson  ct  al,  1958) .  However,  depression  of  the  thymus  weight  to  below 
the  control  levels  in  the  larger  mice  from  high  populations  can  only  reflect 
increased  adrenocortical  activity.  The  mean  thymus  weights  of  mice  from 
the  intermediate  populations  were  greater  than  those  of  the  controls  or 
experimental  mice  from  high  populations  in  the  16-19-gm.  body  weight 
range.  Data  from  the  X-zone  indicate  that  the  secretion  of  androgen  (or 
at  least  its  activity)  was  inhibited  to  the  same  degree  in  the  intermediate 
and  high  populations,  whereas  adrenal  weights  and  width  of  the  zona  fas- 
ciculata  were  less  in  the  intermediate  than  in  the  high  populations.  There- 
fore the  greater  thymus  weights  in  mice  weighing  16-19  grams  from  the 
intermediate  populations  may  have  resulted  from  a  less  marked  increase  in 
adrenocortical  activity  than  occurred  in  the  high-density  populations, 
whereas  androgen  secretion  was  depressed  equally  in  populations  of  both 
sizes.  These  results  might  be  interpreted  to  mean  that  at  increased  popula- 
tion densities  the  younger,  and  presumably  subordinate,  animals  are  the 
ones  predominantly  affected  by  increased  density.  Since  these  results  are 
obtained  by  sacrificing  an  entire  population  at  one  time,  such  a  conclusion 
would  be  valid  if  it  were  not  for  the  fact  that  the  evidence  indicates  that  the 
increase  in  pituitary-adrenocortical  activity  involved  all  weight  groups  and 
therefore  all  ages.  Nevertheless,  as  we  shall  see  below,  a  few  of  the  heaviest 


284  /.  /.   Christian 

animals,  probably  including  the  dominant  animals,  were  less  affected  by 
increased  density  than  those  in  any  other  weight  group. 

It  was  mentioned  earlier  than  in  response  to  stimuli  which  evoke  in- 
creased adrenocortical  activity  there  is  often  inhibition  of  growth  resulting 
either  from  direct  inhibition  of  growth  by  carbohydrate-active  corticoids, 
by  inhibition  of  the  secretion  of  pituitary  growth  hormone,  or  both.  Evi- 
dence has  been  presented  indicating  that  there  is  inhibition  of  growth  with 
increased  population  density.  Therefore,  one  might  question  the  comparison 
of  organ  weight  data  from  mice  from  populations  of  high  density  with  those 
from  segregated  mice,  as  mice  from  the  high-density  populations  may  be 
older  than  segregated  controls  for  the  same  body  weight.  However,  if  it 
were  possible  to  correct  for  age,  the  differences  with  respect  to  body  weight 
between  high-density  populations  and  segregated  controls  would  be  even 
greater.  Therefore,  comparisons  with  respect  to  body  weight  will  err  on  the 
conservative  side. 

The  zonae  fasciculatae  of  female  mice  of  all  sizes  from  high  populations 
were  appreciably  wider  than  their  controls  as  a  result  of  cellular  hyper- 
plasia and  hypertrophy,  although  the  presumptive  increase  in  adrenocorti- 
cal secretion  was  not  indicated  by  the  thymus  weights.  If  anything,  the 
thymuses  of  females  from  the  experimental  populations  were  heavier  than 
those  of  the  segregated  controls,  possibly  a  reflection  of  a  diminished  secre- 
tion of  the  sex  steroids. 

On  the  other  hand  the  preputial  glands  respond  to  stimulation  by  ACTH 
(Jacot  and  Selye,  1951;  Hess  et  al,  1952,  1953;  Rennels  et  al.,  1953),  and 
the  preputials  of  female  mice  from  high-density  populations  were  heavier 
than  those  from  their  segregated  controls.  Therefore,  it  is  possible  that  the 
increase  in  the  weights  of  the  preputial  glands  of  these  mice  may  have  re- 
sulted from  an  increased  secretion  of  ACTH.  This  problem  will  be  discussed 
in  more  detail  subsequently. 

The  effect  of  increased  population  density  on  adrenocortical  function  of 
voles  {Microtus  pennsylv aniens)  in  freely  growing  confined  populations  has 
also  been  studied.  The  experimental  procedure  used  for  these  populations 
was  essentially  the  same  as  for  the  house  mice  except  that  eosinophil 
coimts  were  used  to  measure  adrenocortical  function  in  the  three  popula- 
tions of  voles  (Louch,  1956).  The  use  of  eosinophil  counts  has  an  obvious 
advantage  over  adrenal  weight  for  assessing  adrenocortical  function  in 
that  the  animals  do  not  have  to  be  sacrificed,  especially  when  various  factors 
contributing  to  variation  in  the  counts  are  taken  into  consideration  ( Louch 
et  al.,  1953) .  In  such  long-term  studies  as  this  with  repeated  counts,  there 
can  be  little  doubt  that  the  eosinophil  counts  reflect  adrenocortical  function 
rather  than  medullary.  There  was  a  significant  negative  correlation  between 


2.  Endocrines  and  Populations  285 

eosinophil  counts  and  population  density,  indicating  a  progressive  decline 
decline  in  the  number  of  circulating  eosinophils  with  increasing  population 
density,  but  the  correlation  between  eosinophil  counts  and  population 
density  was  not  significant  in  the  third  population.  The  latter  population, 
however,  never  reached  30  animals  in  size.  The  published  figures  indicate 
a  striking  parallel  between  the  rate  of  population  growth  and  eosinophil 
counts  for  all  three  populations,  and  in  all  three  there  were  significantly 
fewer  circulating  eosinophil  when  the  populations  of  these  voles  were  greater 
than  30  than  when  they  were  less  than  30  (Louch,  1956).  These  results 
indicate  that  adrenocortical  function  increased  progressively  with  increasing 
population  density.  The  variability  in  Louch's  data  reflect  to  some  extent 
the  difficulties  in  obtaining  precise  eosinophil  counts,  even  though  he  was 
fully  aware  of  these  problems  and  took  every  step  possible  to  a\^oid  the 
usual  pitfalls  (Louch  et  al,  1953;  Louch,  1956).  The  problems  involved  in 
using  eosinophil  counts  as  indices  of  adrenocortical  function  have  been  dis- 
cussed fully  elsewhere  (Thorn  et  al,  1953;  Louch  et  al,  1953;  Rosemberg 
et  al,  1954;  Speirs,  1955;  Visscher  and  Halberg,  1955;  Louch,  1956;  and 
earlier  in  this  chapter) .  Handling  will  cause  an  adrenal  medullary  and  corti- 
cal discharge  due  to  fear,  excitement,  and  possibly  rage  in  wild  mammals 
and  therefore  can  effect  eosinophil  counts  (Southwick,  1959) .  Conseciuently 
it  is  possible  for  variability  to  result  from  handling,  as  well  as  by  individual 
and  perhaps  more  importantly,  by  unknown  factors,  unless  appropriate 
precautions  are  taken  (Louch,  1958).  However,  in  spite  of  these  problems, 
eosinophil  counts  offer  a  promising  means  of  investigating  adrenocortical- 
population  density  relationships  until  simple,  more  direct  tests  become 
available.  Probably  the  best  procedure  at  the  present  time  is  to  follow  the 
population  with  eosinophil  counts  and  to  substantiate  the  changes  with 
adrenal  weights  at  the  termination  of  the  study.  Louch's  results  provide 
evidence  of  increased  adrenocortical  function  in  voles  with  increased  popu- 
lation density. 

Li  a  similar  experiment,  a  marked  increase  in  adrenal  weight,  as  an  index 
of  cortical  activity  was  found  in  both  male  and  female  voles  (Microtus 
pennsijlvanicus)  from  a  freely  growing  confined  population  which  had 
reached  its  maximum  size  (Christian,  1959b) .  The  adrenals  of  mature  male 
voles  were  increased  39.6%,  and  those  of  mature  females  36.6%  over  their 
segregated  controls.  It  was  pointed  out  earlier  that  adrenal  weight-body 
size  relationships  may  vary  with  species  and  with  sex;  therefore,  if  animals 
covering  a  wide  range  of  sizes  are  to  be  used,  the  correct  relationship  in 
these  must  be  determined.  It  was  found  that  in  male  voles  over  115  mm 
long,  adrenal  weight  did  not  vary  with  further  increases  in  the  size  of  the 
animal,  therefore  the  absolute  adrenal  weights  could  be  used  for  compara- 


286  /.  /.  Christian 

tive  purposes.  However,  in  female  voles  adrenal  weight  increased  with  in- 
crease in  body  size;  therefore  for  females  it  was  necessary  to  use  relative 
adrenal  weights  for  purposes  of  comparison. 

3.  Reproduction 

a.  Male.  Reproductive  function  was  depressed  in  proportion  to  increases 
in  population  size  in  both  male  and  female  house  mice  from  populations  of 
fixed  size.  The  results  of  experiments  with  freely  growing  confined  popula- 
tions of  house  mice  and  voles  also  show  a  progressive  decrease  in  reproduc- 
tive function  with  increasing  density.  We  have  seen  that  there  was  a  delay 
in  the  onset  of  puberty  in  male  house  mice  from  freely  growing  populations 
of  high  density,  as  demonstrated  by  the  delayed  involution  of  the  adrenal 
X-zone  and  the  development  of  the  testes,  seminal  vesicles,  and  preputial 
glands.  The  size  of  the  gonads  and  sex  accessories  was  less  than  their  con- 
trols from  segregated  mice  of  all  but  the  heaviest  body  weight.  Rapid  de- 
velopment of  the  seminal  vesicles  and  preputial  glands  did  not  begin  until  a 
body  weight  of  16  gm.  was  reached,  which  coincides  exactly  with  the  be- 
ginning of  X-zone  involution  in  animals.  These  results  strengthen  the  con- 
clusion that  androgen  secretion  was  delayed.  In  the  segregated  controls,  the 
sex  accessories  had  begun  rapid  growth  by  the  time  the  animals  had  reached 
a  weight  of  13  gm.  These  results  were  confirmed  by  histologic  examination 
of  the  testes.  The  size  of  the  testes  primarily  reflected  the  advancement  of 
spermatogenesis  and  the  development  of  the  seminiferous  tubules.  Sperma- 
togenesis was  inhibited  to  the  same  degree  as  the  sex  accessories  (Christian, 
1956) . 

Male  mice  from  freely  growing  populations  of  intermediate  size  exhibited 
an  inhibition  of  the  reproductive  organs  which  was  approximately  inter- 
mediate between  that  exhibited  by  the  males  from  the  high  populations  and 
the  segregated  controls  (Christian,  1956). 

It  is  of  additional  interest  that  testes  and  sex  accessories  of  male  mice 
weighing  28  gm.  or  more,  the  heaviest  weight  group  in  the  experimental 
populations,  were  about  the  same  size  as  those  of  the  isolated  controls. 
Furthermore,  there  was  no  inhibition  of  spermatogenesis  in  these  animals. 
In  general,  this  weight  group  contained  the  dominant  animals  in  the  four 
high-density  populations  examined  and  was  represented  by  a  total  of  21 
animals.  These  mice  represented  the  initially  introduced  males  and  those 
males  from  the  first  litter  or  two  that  had  established  their  dominance  early 
in  the  histories  of  the  populations.  These  observations  are  confirmed  by 
Crowcroft  and  Rowe  (1957),  who  observed  that  the  productive  females  in 
in  confined  populations  of  house  mice  were  the  introduced  animals  or  those 
from  their  first  few  litters.  Detailed  observations  relating  the  weights  of 


2.  Endocrines  and  Populations  287 

reproductive  organs  to  specific  animals  were  not  made,  but  it  is  likely  that 
these  results  parallel  those  from  populations  of  limited  size  in  which  it  was 
found  that  the  weights  of  the  reproductive  organs  were  greatest  in  the 
socially  dominant  animals.  We  have  indicated  that  the  weights  of  the 
accessory  reproductive  organs  appear  to  be  the  most  sensitive  indications 
of  changes  in  population  density  as  well  as  social  pressures  and  differences 
in  rank.  This  also  appears  to  be  the  case  in  these  experiments  with  freely 
growing  populations.  Reproductive  function  of  male  mice  has  not  been 
examined  in  detail,  usually  not  at  all,  but  other  investigators  studying 
freely  growing  populations,  although  Strecker  and  Emlen  (1953)  did  find 
all  the  males  in  a  self-limited  population  with  epididymal  sperm  6  months 
after  population  growth  had  ceased. 

It  might  reasonably  be  asked  why,  during  the  6  months  or  so  that  most 
of  these  populations  lasted,  there  was  not  adaptation  to  the  situation  with 
a  diminution  of  the  effects  of  population  density  on  the  adaptive  responses 
of  the  adrenals  and  reproductive  organs.  Brown  (1953)  and  Southwick 
(1955b)  have  both  pointed  out  that  in  a  well-stabilized  social  hierarchy  of 
mice  there  is  a  relatively  low  level  of  fighting,  usually  used  as  an  indication 
of  the  amount  of  sociopsychologic  pressure  and  interaction,  but  that  when 
shifts  or  disruptions  in  the  hierarchy  occur,  due  to  the  maturation  of  new 
individuals  or  death  of  old  ones,  social  pressures  increase,  as  indicated  by  an 
increase  in  the  amount  of  fighting.  Southwick  (1955b)  has  pointed  out  that 
these  factors  are  constantly  disrupting  the  social  order  in  rapidly  growing 
populations,  therefore,  as  the  population  increases  it  is  inevitable  that  social 
pressures  increase  apace.  Comparable  results  have  been  observed  in  other 
freely  growing  populations,  but  there  is  not  necessarily  fighting  (Christian, 
1956,  1959a,  b).  Furthermore,  female  mice  become  particularly  aggressive 
prior  to  parturition  (Brown,  1953;  Crowcroft,  1954),  and  so  the  total 
amount  of  aggressiveness  contributed  by  females  would  tend  to  increase 
with  the  population.  For  these  reasons  it  appears  that  the  amount  of  social 
pressure  in  a  population  will  increase  with  the  size  of  population.  It  has 
been  observed  also  that  the  self-limited  maxunum  size  varies  greatly  from 
population  to  population  (Southwick,  1955a,  b;  Christian,  1956)  appar- 
ently as  a  function  of  the  amount  of  social  pressure  within  the  population, 
the  variation  being  contributed  by  individual  differences,  the  stability  of 
the  social  structure,  and  similar  factors.  For  example,  Southwick  (1955b) 
has  described  the  individual  differences  between  males  in  the  amount  of 
territory  they  will  fight  over.  Southwick  ( 1955b)  has  discussed  in  consider- 
able detail  some  of  the  factors  involved  in  the  composition  of  social  compe- 
tition between  mice.  One  population  has  been  observed  in  which  the  social 
order  of  the  population  was  disrupted  at  about  half  the  estimated  maximum 
size  by  the  death  of  an  old,  tyrannical  male  (Christian,  1956,  and  unpub- 


288  /•  /.   Christian 

lished) .  Severe  fighting  broke  out  in  this  population  and  the  growth  of  the 
population  ceased  completely  until  social  order  was  reestablished  6  weeks 
later.  The  cessation  of  growth  resulted  from  a  complete  cessation  of  all 
aspects  of  reproductive  activity.  These  considerations,  plus  the  fact  that 
the  physiologic  responses  are  equivalent  in  populations  of  maximum  self- 
limited  size  irrespective  of  the  number  of  mice  (Christian,  1956),  make  it 
likely  that  the  growth  of  populations  of  house  mice,  and  possibly  voles,  is 
limited  by  the  total  amount  of  social  pressure  rather  than  by  the  number 
of  animals. 

The  reproductive  competence  of  males  has  not  been  examined  in  detail 
for  any  species  from  freely  growing  populations  other  than  house  mice ;  and 
no  studies  have  adequately  explored  the  problem.  In  most  studies  on  popu- 
lation density  the  criteria  of  male  fertility  are  position  of  the  testes,  gross 
size  of  the  testes,  visibility  of  the  epididymal  tubules,  and  occasionally  the 
presence  or  absence  of  sperm  in  the  epididymes  (Strecker  and  Emlen,  1953; 
Brown,  1953 ;  Southwick,  1955a;  Crowcroft  and  Rowe,  1957) .  These  criteria 
are  actually  poor  indicators  of  relative  fertility  and  only  suggest  whether 
or  not  an  animal  is  mature.  Detailed  morphologic  studies  provide  more 
evidence  of  fertility  (Christian,  1956),  but  they  do  not  provdde  conclusive 
evidence.  No  information  on  subtle  changes  in  fertility  is  provided  by  any 
of  these  criteria.  The  ability  of  males  to  fertilize  females  with  respect  to 
population  density  has  not,  to  my  knowledge,  been  investigated. 

Many  influences  which  depress  reproductive  activity  cause  striking 
degenerative  changes  in  the  germinal  cells  in  the  testes  (Selye,  1947).  The 
formation  of  giant  cells  from  cells  of  the  spermatogenic  series  is  a  common 
indicator  of  such  changes  (Selye,  1950;  Steinberger  and  Dixon,  1959). 
Furthermore,  influences  which  can  produce  severe  degenerative  changes 
or  inhibit  spermatogenesis  completely  can  produce  more  subtle  changes  if 
the  damaging  stimulus  is  less  severe  (Steinberger  and  Dixon,  1959).  It 
seems  likely  that  the  conspicuous  changes  that  occur  in  the  testes  of  house 
mice  as  a  result  of  increasing  population  density  [a  delay  in  the  onset  of 
spermatogenesis,  reduction  in  the  number  of  mature  spermatoza,  and 
formation  of  giant  cells  (Christian,  1951,  1956)]  can  also  be  extended  to 
include  less  obvious  abnormalities  of  the  sperm,  such  as  decreased  motility 
and  viability.  There  also  may  be  a  decrease  in  the  nmnber  of  sperm  pro- 
duced, and  therefore  in  the  number  of  sperm  in  the  ejaculate.  All  these 
factors  presumably  affect  fertility  (Chang  and  Pincus,  1951).  To  these 
must  be  added  the  likelihood  of  an  altered  medium  for  the  sperm  which 
would  be  suggested  by  the  changes  in  the  sex  accessories  associated  with 
testicular  changes,  as  indicated  by  the  decline  in  their  weights  (Burrows, 
1949;  Lutwak-Mann  et  al.,  1949;  Leathem,  1950;  Mann,  1954) .  Atrophy  of 
sex  accessories  carries  the  implication  that  their  secretory  products  may  be 


2.  Endocrines  and  Populations  280 

abnormal,  deficient,  or  decreased  in  amount,  and  that  the  composition  of 
the  ejaculate  will  in  all  probability  not  provide  an  optimal  medium  for 
sperm  (Burrows,  1949;  Lutwak-Mann  et  al.,  1949;  Cavazos  and  Melampy, 
1954).  Therefore  it  is  possible  that  male  fertility  is  depressed  appreciably 
by  increased  population  density,  although  the  gross  examination  or  rela- 
tively minor  changes  in  weight  would  provide  no  indication  of  such  a  reduc- 
tion in  male  fertility.  Male  fertility  is  assessed  in  most  studies  on  small 
mammals  by  (1)  the  position  of  the  testes,  (2)  whether  or  not  the  epididy- 
mal  tubules  are  clearly  visible,  or  (3)  possibly  by  epididymal  smears  to 
determine  the  presence  or  absence  of  spermatozoa  (Southwick,  195oa; 
Louch,  1956;  Strecker  and  Emlen,  1953;  Crowcroft  and  Rowe,  1957). 
Degenerative  changes  can  be  detected  in  testicular  smears  (Christian, 
1950a,  1951) ,  but  with  much  less  assurance  than  properly  prepared,  stained, 
and  critically  examined  sections  of  the  testes.  Morphologic  studies  of  the 
testes,  if  properly  done,  can  be  extremely  revealing,  but  the  weights  of  the 
testes  and  accessory  organs,  while  providing  a  useful  index  of  reproductive 
function  in  general,  do  not  tell  whether  or  not  the  male  is  actually  fertile 
unless  inhibition  is  severe.  In  the  absence  of  good  evidence  to  the  contrary 
it  is  generally  assumed  that  the  males  in  a  population  are  fertile — an  as- 
sumption that  may  be  misleading.  Studies  of  male  fertility  in  relation  to 
population  density  are  needed  that  make  use  of  sperm  counts,  determina- 
tion of  the  motility  and  viability  of  the  sperm,  and  possibly  direct  determi- 
nation of  fertility  by  mating  with  proven  females.  Admittedly  such  a  pro- 
gram would  present  problems,  but  they  are  by  no  means  insurmountable. 
Furthermore,  once  the  techniques  are  worked  out  for  obtaining  ejaculates 
from  the  males  in  populations  of  small  mammals,  it  should  be  a  valuable 
and  useful  procedure  for  routinely  assessing  male  fertility  and  reproductive 
competence  in  freely  growing  populations  of  small  mammals.  But  it  is  in- 
correct to  assume  that  male  fertility  is  not  affected  by  increased  population 
density,  especially  in  view  of  the  evidence  indicating  that  the  secretion  of 
testicular  androgens  is  extremely  sensitive  to  changes  in  population  density 
or  to  social  rank. 

Southwick  (1955a)  observed  a  decrease  in  fertility  in  female  mice  from 
populations  which  were  slowly  declining  from  peak  levels.  He  also  noted,  as 
have  others  (Southern,  1948;  Calhoun,  1949),  that  in  populations  of  high 
density  or  with  poor  social  organization  (Calhoun,  1949,  1950)  that  "copu- 
lation pressure"  on  females  in  estrus  was  high;  that  is,  a  number  of  males 
gathered  around  her  and  attempted  to  copulate  although  they  were  fre- 
quently pushed  off  by  others,  so  that  the  mean  copulation  time  per  male 
was  reduced.  "Copulation  pressure"  was  used  as  a  possible  explanation 
for  the  decline  in  fertility.  This  explanation  overlooks  several  facts.  The 
populations  were  progressively  declining  from  peak  levels  and  did  so  for  six 


290  J.  J.   Christian 

months,  and  so  did  pregnancy  rates  and  fecundity  rates  (correspondence  in 
the  dechnes  in  these  latter  two  was  used  as  an  argument  against  the  possi- 
bility of  increased  intra-uterine  mortality) .  It  is  therefore  unlikely  that  a 
steadily  declining  fertility  can  be  explained  by  copulation  pressure  in  a 
constantly  decreasing  number  of  animals.  Male  fertility  was  assessed  by 
uncritical  procedures  and  female  fertility  by  performance.  Assessing  fer- 
tility by  female  performance  of  course  does  not  provide  a  way  to  assess  the 
possible  role  of  depressed  male  fertility.  It  seems  much  more  likely  that 
both  male  and  female  fertility  were  depressed.  Crowcroft  and  Rowe  (1957) 
noted  a  decline  in  fertility  in  female  house  mice  with  increasing  density  as 
well  as  during  the  period  of  asymptotic  stability  in  populations  of  house 
mice.  These  authors  indicate  the  failure  of  population  growth  resulted  en- 
tirely from  inhibition  of  female  reproductive  function.  Louch  (1956)  was 
unable  to  correlate  diminished  fertility  with  "copulatory  pressure,"  al- 
though it  was  observed  in  his  population  of  voles.  We  have  seen  that  the 
females  in  extremely  dense  populations  became  pregnant  but  failed  to  bear 
litters  and  that  recovery  from  the  effects  of  chronically  depressed  fertility 
was  extremely  slow  (Christian  and  LeMunyan,  1958).  Increasing  age  may 
have  been  a  factor  in  these  freely  growing  populations,  especially  if  fertility 
was  already  partially  curtailed. 

This  discussion  is  meant  neither  to  be  a  criticism  of  the  observations 
made  by  these  investigators  nor  to  provide  the  correct  explanation  for 
them.  Rather,  these  examples  have  been  used  to  indicate  pitfalls  in  experi- 
mental procedures  presently  in  general  use  to  assess  male  fertility  and  the 
effects  of  various  factors  on  fertility  in  populations  of  small  mammals,  and 
to  call  attention  to  reasonable  and  perhaps  more  probable  explanations 
other  than  those  usually  provided.  The  literature  contains  many  examples 
which  could  have  been  used.  Finally,  it  should  be  noted  that  the  popula- 
tions in  Southwick's  studies  had  reached  peak  densities  and  had  been  de- 
clining slowly  for  6  months  before  the  animals  were  examined,  so  it  would 
be  difficult  to  determine  the  effects  of  increasing  density  on  fertility  in  mice 
of  either  sex.  The  results  of  those  studies  made  during  the  period  of  increase 
of  a  population  or  at  the  time  it  reached  maximum  density  are  not  com- 
parable to  studies  made  so  much  later,  and  it  is  not  correct  to  extrapolate 
from  one  to  the  other,  as  was  done  in  several  instances.  The  results  of  other 
experiments  in  which  male  fecundity  was  assessed  during  the  period  of 
population  increase  indicate  that  male  fertility,  in  terms  of  the  whole 
population,  has  a  striking  negative  correlation  with  density  (Christian, 
1956,  1959a,  b;  Crowcroft  and  Rowe,  1957) .  The  secretion  of  gonadotropins 
is  apparently  depressed  with  increasing  pituitary-adrenocortical  activity 
in  the  greater  proportion  of  subordinate  animals  associated  with  increases 
in  density.  Evidence  has  been  presented  which  shows  that  increased  adreno- 


2.  Endocrines  and  Populations  291 

cortical  activity  in  mice  results  in  an  increase  in  the  secretion  of  weakly 
androgenic  steroids,  and  that  these  can  depress  the  secretion  of  gonado- 
tropms. 

h.  Female.  Reproductive  competence  of  female  mammals  can  be  assessed 
grossly  more  easily  than  that  of  males  diu-ing  studies  of  growing  popula- 
tions simply  because  the  number  of  young  produced  can  be  counted.  Some 
comments  have  been  made  about  the  relationships  of  female  fertility  and 
reproduction  to  population  density  in  animals  from  freely  growing  popula- 
tions. Most  of  the  pertinent  evidence  indicates  that  fertility  and  reproduc- 
tion in  female  mammals  is  depressed  by  increased  density.  Two  kinds  of 
information  has  been  gathered  on  female  productivity  in  relation  to  popula- 
tion density.  In  one  case  the  number  of  young  born  at  intervals  throughout 
the  history  of  the  population  has  been  determined,  and  in  the  other  female 
fertility  is  assessed  by  external  examination  of  the  vagina,  which  is  closed 
by  a  thin  membrane  during  anestrus  in  many  species,  or  by  examination  of 
the  animals  at  autopsy.  Information  of  both  kinds  is  desirable,  although 
neither  is  sufficient  by  itself. 

It  has  been  shown  in  studies  with  house  mice  and  voles  in  freely  growing 
confined  populations  from  which  adequate  data  are  available  that  the  birth 
rate  for  mature  or  potentially  mature  females  in  the  population  declines 
steadily  with  increasing  population  size  (Clarke,  1955;  Christian,  1956, 
1959b,  1961;  Crowcroft,  and  Rowe,  1957).  The  decline  in  the  number  of 
young  born  per  female  appears  to  decline  approximately  linearly  as  the  logio 
of  the  population  increases  (Christian,  1959b), 

The  declines  in  productivity  apparently  resulted  from  suppression  of  all 
phases  of  reproductive  functions.  Crowcroft  and  Rowe  (1957)  showed 
there  was  complete  suppression  of  estrus,  as  indicated  by  closure  of  the 
vaginal  orifice,  in  an  increasingly  larger  proportion  of  females  as  the  size  of 
a  population  increased.  It  was  shown  that  closure  of  the  vaginal  orifice  was 
a  reliable  index  of  fertility  in  female  house  mice  in  these  experiments  by 
the  very  high  correlation  between  the  condition  of  the  vaginal  orifice  and 
the  development  of  the  reproductive  organs.  In  these  populations  the 
majority  of  the  young  were  produced  by  the  introduced  females  initially  or 
by  females  from  the  first  few  litters.  These  results  correspond  to  those  from 
populations  of  limited  size  in  which  it  was  indicated  that  the  reproductive 
competence  of  female  mice  was  related  to  their  social  status.  Similar  results 
were  also  observed  in  confined  populations  of  Norway  rats  (Calhoun,  1949, 
1950) .  Reproductive  function  was  completely  suppressed  in  the  majority 
of  female  born  into  these  populations. 

These  results  are  comparable  to  those  from  experiments  with  freely 
growing  populations  of  house  mice  (Christian,  1956),  thus  indicating  that 
reproductive  function  in  female  mice  was  depressed  for  all  body-weight 


292  J.  J.  Christian 

groups.  In  these  experiments  the  weights  of  the  preputial  glands  of  mice 
from  populations  of  high  density  were  significantly  greater  than  those  from 
segregated  controls.  For  their  stimulation  to  full  development  and  function, 
the  preputial  glands  depend  primarily  on  the  more  potent  androgens  (Bur- 
rows, 1949),  but  the  weakly  androgenic  steroids  (Huggins  et  al.,  1955), 
ACTH  (Jacot  and  Selye,  1951;  Asling  et  al.,  1951),  and  pituitary  growth 
hormone  (Huggins  et  al.,  1955)  all  have  a  mild  stimulatory  action  on  the 
preputials.  The  evidence  for  the  effects  of  estrogens  on  the  preputials  is 
conflicting.  On  the  one  hand,  estrogens  appear  to  have  a  mild  stimulatory 
action  (Burrows,  1949;  Beyler  and  Szego,  1954)  whereas  other  experiments 
indicate  that  they  inhibit  the  preputials  (Rennels  et  al.,  1953).  Since  other 
evidence  from  the  experiments  with  mouse  populations  indicates  that  the 
production  of  sex  steroids  by  the  gonads  and  growth  hormone  by  the  pitui- 
tary is  inhibited  at  high  densities,  it  seems  probable  that  the  increased  size 
of  the  preputials  in  these  intact  females  must  have  been  due  to  increased 
ACTH  and  possibly  increased  adrenal  C19  steroids.  In  any  event  the  data 
on  the  preputials  are  strongly  suggestive  of  increased  pituitary-adrenocorti- 
cal  function.  Attention  should  be  directed  to  the  fact  that  the  preputials 
were  appreciably  larger  than  those  from  segregated  mice  with  full  reproduc- 
tive competence.  Therefore  there  is  no  reason  whatsoever  to  implicate  estro- 
gens in  these  effects ;  so  ACTH  and  adrenal  androgens  must  be  responsible 
for  stimulating  preputial  development  to  well  above  the  control  levels. 
Presumably  the  levels  of  estrogens  declined  in  these  females  as  a  result  of 
a  decreased  secretion  of  pituitary  gonadotropins  in  association  with  in- 
creased secretion  of  ACTH.  These  conclusions  were  supported  by  the  fact 
that  there  was  also  (1)  a  marked  increase  in  the  proportion  of  females  of 
adult  size  with  infantile  uteri  and  ovaries,  and  (2)  a  20%  decline  m 
number  of  females  with  mature  reproductive  organs  that  were  also  preg- 
nant, (3)  a  13%  decrease  in  the  mean  number  of  viable  embryos  per 
pregnancy,  and  (4)  a  58%  increase  in  the  number  of  resorbing  embryos  per 
pregnancy.  Thus  there  was  diminished  fertility  and  increased  losses  between 
o\'ulation  and  implantation,  and  implantation  and  birth.  Decreased  fertility 
was  shown  in  some  animals  by  a  total  inhibition  of  reproduction  and  in 
others  by  a  markedly  diminished  number  of  viable  embryos  per  pregnancy. 
These  results  agree  with  those  of  Crowcroft  and  Rowe  (1957).  The  same 
phenomena  apparently  occurred  in  populations  of  voles  (Clarke,  1955; 
Louch,  1956;  Christian,  1959b) .  Birth  rates  declined  steadily  as  the  popula- 
tions increased  for  house  mice  and  voles  (Christian,  1959b,  1961).  There- 
fore it  seems  that  reproductive  function  is  inhibited  in  female  mice  and  voles 
in  proportion  to  population  density,  either  in  terms  of  the  proportion  of 
reproductively  competent  adult  females  in  a  population  or  of  the  degree  of 
reprodu(•ti^'e  function  in   individuals.    Stated  another  way,   reproductive 


2.  Endocrines  and  Populations  293 

function  in  female  mice  and  voles  is  reciprocally  related  to  population 
density.  The  degree  of  inhibition  apparently  may  vary  from  none  to  com- 
plete in  the  females  of  a  given  population,  within  limits  depending  on  social 
relationships  of  the  particular  individuals. 

We  have  already  criticized  the  concept  of  "copulation  pressure"  which 
has  been  used  to  explain  the  decline  in  female  fertility  seen  in  populations 
of  high  density.  As  an  explanation  it  is  inadequate  because  it  does  not 
coincide  with  the  facts,  even  in  those  cases  in  which  it  has  been  used  as  an 
explanation  (Southwick,  1955b).  Furthermore,  no  correlation  could  be 
shown  between  "copulation  pressure"  and  fertility  in  other  studies  (Louch, 
1956) .  In  the  light  of  those  studies  in  which  female  reproductive  function 
was  more  precisely  assessed,  it  appears  that  reproductive  function  was  at 
least  partially  inhibited  in  those  populations  in  which  "copulation  pressure" 
was  used  to  explain  the  decline  in  fertility,  and  that  the  means  used  to  assay 
female  fertility  were  not  sufficiently  sensitive.  "Copulation  pressure"  con- 
ceivably could  have  an  effect  on  female  reproductive  performance,  but 
until  the  problem  is  reexamined  more  critically  it  must  remain  an  unaccept- 
able concept,  especially  in  view  of  the  preponderance  of  evidence  indicating 
that  reproductive  function  is  inhibited,  either  individually  or  on  a  popula- 
tion-wide basis,  as  part  of  the  adaptive  responses  to  increased  density.  A 
broad  view  of  this  evidence  suggests  considerable  variability  from  popula- 
tion to  population  in  the  degree  of  reproductive  inhibition  with  respect  to 
the  numbers  of  females  involved  as  well  as  to  the  magnitude  of  inhibition 
in  particular  individuals.  These  variations  are  evidently  related  to  social 
factors,  and  this  aspect  of  the  problem  needs  examination  in  greater  detail 
than  heretofore. 

Prenatal  mortality  of  the  fetuses  was  an  appreciable  factor  in  reducing 
the  number  of  births  in  dense  populations,  presumably  largely  owing  to  a 
suppression  of  gonadotropins,  but  also  possibly  to  the  effects  of  increased 
adrenal  corticoids  and  androgens.  There  seems  to  be  some  kind  of  "dual 
response"  to  density  in  depressing  female  productivity.  Judging  from  the 
results  described  in  this  and  preceding  sections,  it  appears  that  the  least 
"dose-response"  effect  occurs  when  fertility  is  partially  diminished,  as  by 
decreased  numbers  of  ova  and  increased  losses  between  ovulation  and  birth, 
without  a  total  suppression  on  the  ability  to  bear  young.  A  greater  effect 
apparently  is  total  inhibition  of  estrus  and  reproduction  in  potentially 
mature  animals.  Delayed  attainment  of  maturity  probably  reflects  partial 
inhibition.  The  possible  mechanisms  involved  in  producing  intra-uterine 
mortality  have  been  discussed  in  an  earlier  section. 

c.  Litter  survival.  A  marked  decline  in  litter  survival  with  increasing 
population  density  was  observed  in  all  but  a  few  experiments  with  freely 
growing  populations  of  house  mice  or  voles  (Rtrecker  and  Emlen,  1953; 


294  /•  /•  Christian 

Brown,  1953;  Clarke,  1955;  Southwick,  1955a,  b;  Christian,  1956;  Louch, 
1956).  Litter  survival  apparently  declines  approximately  linearly  as  the 
logarithms  of  the  population  increases  (Christian,  1959b).  However,  litter 
mortality  is  not  always  a  factor  in  diminishing  the  rate  of  population 
growth.  Crowcroft  and  Rowe  (1957)  found  very  little  mortality  of  young 
mice  in  their  populations  and  furthermore  were  able  to  attribute  all  the 
observed  mortality  to  interference  on  their  own  part  in  making  routine 
censuses.  The  decline  in  productivity  in  their  populations  was  due  almost 
entirely  to  prenatal  loss  or  complete  inhibition  of  all  reproductive  activity. 
It  has  been  suggested  (cf.  above)  that  diminished  fertility  probably  was 
more  of  a  factor  than  was  realized  in  several  other  investigations;  neverthe- 
less litter  mortality  has  been  an  unportant  factor  in  many  populations. 
Decreased  litter  survival  may  be  due  to  diminished  lactation  (Christian, 
1956;  Christian  and  LeMunyan,  1958)  or  to  behavioral  factors  (Brown, 
1953;  Strecker  and  Emlen,  1953;  Southwick,  1955b). 

Partial  inhibition  of  lactation  with  increased  density  was  described 
earlier.  The  results  suggested  that  lactation,  controlled  by  complex  endo- 
crine mechanisms,  was  partially  inhibited  owing  to  inhibition  of  gonado- 
tropin secretion  and  a  resultant  diminished  production  of  estrogens  and 
progestins.  Lactation  therefore  reflected  population  density  in  a  similar 
fashion  to  other  reproductive  functions.  It  was  also  pointed  out  that  the 
progeny  nursed  by  mothers  subjected  to  increased  density  were  profoundly 
affected  by  the  inhibition  of  lactation.  Experiments  with  freely  growing 
populations  of  house  mice  suggest  that  deficient  lactation  may  be  an  im- 
portant factor  in  decreasing  litter  survival  (Christian,  1956).  These  young 
were  stunted  and  were  weaned  early.  At  autopsy  their  stomachs  contained 
little  or  no  milk.  Calhoun  (1949,  1950)  suggested  that  the  socially  sub- 
ordinate rats  in  freely  growing  confined  populations  were  incapable  of 
raising  then*  young,  even  though  they  were  protected  from  disturbance. 
He  indicated  that  physiologic  and  psychologic  disturbance  in  the  sub- 
ordinate females  affected  the  young  through  either  poor  fetal  nutrition  or  a 
breakdown  of  maternal  instincts. 

Dimmished  lactation  may  have  been  an  appreciable  factor  in  reducing 
the  survival  of  young  in  the  above  experiments,  and  diminished  lactation 
is  a  logical  consequence  of  a  generalized  inhibition  of  reproductive  function 
presumably  due  to  diminished  gonadotropin  secretion. 

Decreased  litter  survival  was  attributed  entirely  to  social  or  behavioral 
factors  in  a  number  of  experiments  with  freely  growing  populations  of 
house  mice  or  voles.  Brown  (1953),  Southwick  (1955b),  and  Louch  (1956) 
related  litter  survival  to  the  type  of  nest  constructed  and  maintained. 
Brown  and  Louch  each  noted  that  litter  survival  was  good  so  long  as  the 
female  mice  or  voles  were  able  to  maintain  covered  or  bowl  types  of  nests, 


2.  Endocrines  and  Populations  295 

but  that  this  was  frequently  impossible  because  of  destruction  of  the  nests 
and  interference  with  the  attempts  of  the  females  to  maintain  or  reconstruct 
them.  Only  49  of  the  72  attempts  to  build  and  maintain  either  of  these  two 
types  of  nest  were  successful  in  these  populations  (Brown,  1953).  South- 
wick  (1955b)  noted  a  similar  relationship  between  the  type  of  nest  and 
litter  survival,  but  he  also  found  that  litter  survival  was  good  in  bowl  or 
covered  nests  regardless  of  the  number  or  sex  of  the  adults  which  were  also 
present.  Litter  survival  was  zero  with  no  nests.  These  and  Brown's  (1953) 
results  imply  that  not  all  the  females  were  prevented  from  maintaining 
proper  nests  and  nourishing  then*  litters  irrespective  of  the  conditions  and 
interference.  These  results  are  revealing,  as  they  suggest  strongly  that  more 
is  involved  in  litter  mortality  in  these  experiments  than  simple  interference 
or  the  type  of  nest  maintained.  Similar  results  were  noted  in  other  experi- 
ments (Christian,  1956)  except  that  in  this  case  even  those  with  no  nests 
sometimes  survived.  In  these  latter  experiments  it  was  also  noted  that  if 
litter  mortality  occurred  when  the  populations  were  less  than  one-half  of 
maximum  density,  it  occurred  within  the  first  four  postnatal  days  and 
might  be  attributed  to  interference  which  resulted  in  too  much  movement 
of  the  litter  by  the  mothers.  Of  additional  importance  is  the  fact  that  litter 
survival  declined  regularly  with  increasing  density  and  that  this  began  to 
occur  well  before  interference  could  be  significant.  If  the  young  survived 
the  first  4  days  at  these  lower  densities,  they  almost  always  were  success- 
fully weaned.  However,  at  higher  densities  the  increased  litter  mortality 
usually  occurred  after  the  young  were  10  days  old.  Until  this  age  survival 
equaled  that  seen  at  lower  densities.  These  facts  suggest  that  the  defect  was 
primarily  nutritional,  even  though  there  still  were  some  deaths  of  young 
less  than  4  days  old  attributable  to  interference.  The  age  of  death  of  the 
young  is  not  known  for  most  of  the  reported  experiments,  so  that  this 
distinction  was  not  possible.  However,  the  fact  that  crowding  and  inter- 
ference was  not  a  factor  in  litters  with  proper  nests  suggests  that  perhaps 
the  behavioral  changes  which  resulted  in  poor  nest  maintenance  may  be 
based  on  an  underlying  alteration  in  maternal  behavior,  possibly  due  to 
endocrine  changes.  Admittedly  this  is  speculative,  but  the  available  evi- 
dence suggests  some  such  explanation.  The  problem  certainly  must  be 
investigated  m  greater  detail.  It  is  possible  that  the  capacity  to  lactate 
properly  and  the  abilitj^  to  maintain  proper  nests  are  closely  related  by  a 
common  causal  basis.  However,  until  evidence  for  or  against  such  a  hy- 
pothesis is  available,  it  must  be  assumed  that  postnatal  litter  mortality  at 
high  population  densities  may  result  from  (1)  inhibition  of  lactation,  (2) 
interference  by  other  animals,  or  (3)  changes  in  maternal  behavior,  which 
apparently  decreases  with  increasing  population  density. 

We  are  inclined  to  the  view  that  interference  of  various  sorts  probably 


296  /.  ./.   Christian 

plays  a  more  or  less  constant,  probably  unimportant,  role  in  causing  litter 
mortality  at  all  levels  of  population  density,  whereas  inhibition  of  lactation 
and  changes  in  maternal  behavior  are  density  dependent  and  probably 
reflect  physiologic  (endocrine)  responses  to  changes  in  population  density. 
Southwick  (19o5b)  noted  that  aggressiveness,  as  indicated  by  fights  per 
mouse  per  unit  of  time,  was  reciprocally  related  to  litter  survival  at  high 
densities,  but  that  this  relationship  did  not  necessarily  hold  at  lower 
densities.  On  the  other  hand,  there  was  a  correlation  between  fighting  and 
density  in  only  one  of  three  populations  of  voles  (Louch,  1956).  The  be- 
havioral changes  observed  in  relation  to  litter  survival  may  be  outward 
manifestations  or  "symptoms"  of  more  profound  physiologic  changes  at 
higher  population  densities. 

A  final  consideration  of  importance  is  that  fecundity  and  litter  survival 
are  sufficiently  sensitive  to  changes  in  the  population  to  be  responsive  to  a 
variety  of  different  circumstances  affecting  density.  Litter  survival  was 
markedly  increased  in  a  population  of  house  mice  in  which  there  was  an 
appreciable  mortality  of  adults  (Christian,  19o9b).  The  growth  form  and 
eventual  size  of  the  population  were  comparable  to  those  of  populations 
without  appreciable  adult  mortality.  The  loss  of  adults  in  this  population 
was  compensated  for  by  increased  litter  survival.  Similar  relationships  were 
observed  in  three  populations  of  voles  (Louch,  1956).  There  was  a  signifi- 
cant correlation  between  fertility  and  population  density  in  one  population 
of  voles  in  which  there  was  no  correlation  between  density  and  adult 
mortality.  In  a  second  population  there  was  a  good  correlation  between 
density  and  adult  mortality,  but  none  between  density  and  fertility.  The 
third  population  was  intermediate  between  these  two.  Therefore,  there 
appears  to  be  sufficient  flexibility  in  the  physiologic  adaptive  responses  to 
compensate  for  losses  of  adults  from  populations  of  voles  or  mice.  This 
compensation  apparently  can  occur  at  any  stage  of  the  reproductive  pro- 
cess from  ovulation  to  weaning. 

d.  Effects  of  food  supply.  It  is  often  assumed  without  question  that  food 
shortage  is  responsible  for  limiting  population  growth.  However,  we  have 
seen  that  the  growth  of  a  population  may  be  entirely  self-limited  without  a 
shortage  of  food.  The  role  of  a  limited  food  supply  in  the  regulation  of 
population  growths  has  been  investigated  (Strecker  and  Emlen,  1953; 
Strecker,  1954).  Clarke  (1955),  Christian,  (1955b,  1956,  1959b),  South- 
wick (1955a,  b),  and  Louch  (1956)  have  emphasized  the  fact  that  in 
their  experiments  food  was  always  abundant  and  usually  scattered  so  that 
any  animal  could  obtain  food  irrespective  of  other  animals  present.  Further- 
more, experiments  with  populations  of  fixed  size  indicated  that  inanition 
was  not  a  stimulus  to  increased  adrenocortical  activity  in  house  mice,  nor 
did  it  constitute  a  factor  for  increasing  competition  when  it  (food)  was 


2.  Endocrines  and  Populations  297 

limited  either  in  source  or  amount  (Christian,  1959c).  These  results  are 
consistent  with  those  from  experiments  with  freely  growing  confined 
populations  of  house  mice  or  voles  in  which  competition  for  food  in  every 
case  was  rare  or  absent  (Strecker  and  Emlen,  1953;  Louch,  1956;  South- 
wick,  1955a,  b;  Christian,  1956).  Strecker  and  Emlen  (1953)  provided  two 
confined  populations  with  a  limited  amount  of  food  but  the  growth  of  only 
one  of  these  was  limited  by  the  limited  food  supply.  Growth  of  this  popula- 
tion was  rapid  and  ceased  abruptly  when  the  limit  set  by  the  food  supply 
was  reached.  Growth  ceased  because  reproduction  stopped  abruptly 
with  involution  of  the  reproductive  organs,  especially  of  the  females,  all  of 
which  were  reproductively  quiescent  at  autopsy,  although  the  animals 
otherwise  appeared  to  be  in  excellent  condition  and  usually  excessively  fat. 
Several  points  of  interest  emerge  from  these  experiments.  One  is  that 
population  growth  ceased  very  abruptly  with  a  truncated  growth  curve, 
indicating  that  a  limited  food  supply  does  not  act  as  a  damping  factor,  but 
exerts  its  effects  at  one  point  on  the  growth  curve.  Therefore,  it  is  unlikely 
that  a  typically  sigmoid  growth  curve  would  result  from  a  food  shortage. 
The  second  point  of  interest  is  that  reproduction  was  so  sharply  limited  by 
the  limited  food  supply.  These  results  agree  with  those  from  populations  of 
fixed  size  and  indicate  that  the  food  limitations  completely  inhibited  re- 
production, probably  by  suppressing  the  secretion  of  gonadotropins,  with- 
out producing  increased  activity  of  the  pituitary-adrenocortical  adaptive 
system.  The  last  point  is  that  the  population  immediately  adjusted  to  the 
supply  and  the  general  condition  of  the  mice  was  unaltered  by  the  limitation 
in  food.  The  inhibition  of  reproductive  activity  in  all  the  females  contrasts 
sharply  with  the  results  of  other  experiments  conducted  on  self-limited 
freely  growing  populations  of  mice  or  voles  supplied  with  an  excess  of  food. 
In  the  latter  populations  there  were  always  some  reproductively  active 
females,  although  the  actual  proportion  in  the  population  varied  with  each 
population.  Data  on  adrenal  activity  were  not  collected  in  these  experi- 
ments on  the  efTects  of  food  limitation  on  freely  growing  populations.  It  is 
probable  that  there  would  have  been  density-dependent  increases  in  adreno- 
cortical activity,  but  the  effects  of  food  limitation  were  independent  of  any 
density-dependent  effects.  In  another  series  of  experiments,  the  food  supply 
was  limited  similarly,  but  egress  from  the  population  was  allowed  (Strecker, 
1954) .  In  these  populations  there  was  a  low  constant  rate  of  egress  until 
the  food  limitation  was  reached  and  then  a  high  rate  of  egress  began  and 
continued  until  the  experiment  was  terminated  three  months  later.  The 
egress  apparently  involved  all  segments  of  the  population  except  the 
youngest.  Reproduction  continued  at  a  good  rate  in  this  population  in 
sharp  contrast  to  the  confined  population  without  egress.  The  implications 
of  these  experiments  are  clear:  f1)  food  limitation  can  limit  population 


298  J-  J-  Christian 

growth  by  inhibiting  reproduction,  but  the  growth  curve  becomes  sharply 
truncated;  (2)  if  egress  from  a  population  is  permitted,  food  limitation 
has  no  effect  on  reproduction;  (3)  feeding  must  be  random  and  food  cannot 
have  been  a  factor  increasing  competition,  otherwise  all  the  mice  would  not 
have  been  affected  equally.  Extrapolating  from  these  results  to  natural 
populations,  it  is  evident  that  a  limited  supply  of  food  will  not  affect  repro- 
duction or  otherwise  affect  the  population  so  long  as  emigration  is  possible, 
but  if  emigration  becomes  impossible,  owing  to  a  complete  saturation  of 
the  available  habitat,  reproduction  will  cease  without  any  other  apparent 
effect.  Whether  or  not  there  is  competition  for  food  in  species  other  than 
house  mice  must  be  determined,  as  increased  competition  would  increase 
pituitary-adrenocortical  activity  with  all  its  sequelae.  There  may  be  in- 
creased competition  for  food  in  voles  (Frank,  1953),  although  it  seems  un- 
likely that  in  general  there  will  be  an  increase  in  competition  over  that 
already  present  due  to  social  factors  (cf.  above) .  However,  the  usual  result 
of  emigration  from  a  population  is  increased  mortality:  the  animals  which 
leave  enter  strange  territories  and  very  rapidly  become  mortality  statistics 
(Errington,  1943,  1954b;  Calhoun,  1948;  Davis,  1953).  Physiologic  adap- 
tive mechanisms  apparently  are  always  operative  in  relation  to  population 
density  and  therefore  would  reflect  the  relative  density  whether  or  not  the 
growth  of  a  population  was  limited  by  food,  and  a  distinction  between  food- 
limited  and  self-limited  populations  should  be  possible  by  comparing  the 
reproductive  activity  of  the  populations  and  the  general  conditions  of  the 
individual  animals.  Adrenal  weights  and  other  parameters  of  increased 
pituitary-adrenocortical  activity  should  also  differentiate  between  the  two 
unless  a  population  was  food-limited  at  a  point  close  to  its  maximum  self- 
limited  size. 

These  statements  and  conclusions  are  based  on  the  results  of  experiments 
with  house  mice  and  seem  fairly  conclusive  for  this  species,  possibly  even 
for  voles,  but  it  is  difficult  to  believe  that  food  is  not  a  competitive  factor 
for  all  species  and  for  all  populations.  The  results  of  experiments  to  date 
with  populations  of  fixed  size  or  freely  growing  indicate  that  food  is  not 
even  a  competitive  factor  within  the  existing  social  structure,  as  all  anmials 
are  apparently  equally  affected  irrespective  of  their  social  rank  (Strecker 
and  Emlen,  1953;  Christian  and  LeMunyan,  1958;  Christian,  1959b).  It 
should  be  remembered  that  if  ever  and  when  ever  food  becomes  an  object 
of  competition,  shortages  then  necessarily  will  act  through  the  generalized 
physiologic  adaptive  responses  and  limit  population  growth  in  a  density- 
dependent  fashion  just  as  does  purely  social  competition.  The  net  conclu- 
sion from  these  results  is  that  a  shortage  of  food  per  se  probably  rarely 
limits  population  growth  as  the  peculiar  combination  of  effects  which 
result  from  a  food  shortage  is  seldom  seen.  However,  this  statement  is  not 


2.  Endocrines  and  Populations  299 

to  be  interpreted  to  indicate  that  acute  starvation  cannot  ensue  in  local 
populations  or  that  a  limited  food  supply  is  not  at  times  important.  The 
relationships  between  food,  competition,  reproduction,  and  the  growth  of 
populations  require  much  more  investigation,  especially  for  different 
species.  Until  additional  critical  information  is  available,  any  conclusions 
regarding  food  and  population  growth  must  remain  tentative. 

4.  Growth 

In  a  recent  experiment  with  a  freely  growing  population  of  house  mice, 
it  was  shown  that  there  was  appreciably  inhibited  growth  in  all  but  the 
mice  originally  introduced  and  those  from  the  first  litters  (Christian, 
1961) .  In  this  experiment  all  mice  were  toe-clipped  so  that  their  ages  were 
known.  In  general  the  results  from  the  reproductive  organs  and  adrenal 
glands  confirmed  those  of  earlier  experiments  and  were  even  more  pro- 
nounced because  the  results  could  be  equated  with  age  rather  than  body- 
weight  categories. 

5.  Summary  of  Results  from  Freely  Growing  Experimental  Popu- 
lations 

We  have  seen  that,  for  the  most  part,  freely  growing  populations  of 
house  mice  and  voles  are  self-limited,  primarily  as  a  result  of  a  density- 
dependent  activity  of  physiologic  adaptive  mechanisms.  The  activity  of 
these  mechanisms  is  reflected  in  both  sexes  by  a  progressive  inhibition  of 
reproduction  and  stimulation  of  the  pituitary-adrenocortical  activity  with 
increasing  population  size.  The  results  agree  with  those  obtained  from 
populations  of  fixed  size.  Diminished  reproductive  function  in  the  female 
may  be  apparent  at  any  one  or  all  phases  of  reproductive  activity :  inhibi- 
tion of  estrus,  increased  intra-uterine  mortality,  reduced  nvmibers  of  im- 
planted ova,  increased  postparturient  mortality  due  to  suppression  of 
lactation,  and  possibly  by  diminished  maternal  behavior.  Inhibition  of  male 
reproductive  ability  is  shown  by  decreases  in  the  weights  of  the  sex  acces- 
sories and  depressed  spermatogenesis,  although  the  problem  of  male  fertility 
needs  further  investigation.  The  decreases  in  reproductive  function  in  both 
sexes  apparently  stem  from  decreases  in  the  secretion  of  pituitary  gonado- 
tropins with  a  secondary  decline  in  the  production  of  estrogens,  progestins, 
and  androgens.  Evidence  for  and  against  the  role  of  "increased  copulation 
pressure"  in  diminishing  the  fertility  of  female  mice  and  voles  was  dis- 
cussed. Without  much  more  conclusive  evidence,  it  is  likely  that  "copula- 
tion pressure"  is  an  unimportant  factor  and  that  fertility  is  depressed  by 
increased  density  operating  through  sociopsychologic  and  physiologic  path- 
ways. Increased  adrenocortical  activity  was  shown  by  increased  adrenal 


300  J'  J-   Christian 

weights,  eosinophil  counts,  and  histologic  studies.  Thymus  weights  re- 
flected these  changes.  Increasing  infant  mortality  with  increasing  density 
may  be  due  to  diminished  lactation,  but  may  also  result  from  social  and 
behavioral  factors.  Food  limitation  may  restrict  population  growth  by 
completely  inhibiting  reproduction,  but  is  without  any  other  apparent 
effects. 

Finally,  it  is  evident  that  the  growth  of  confined  populations  of  mice  and 
voles  is  regulated  and  limited  by  density-dependent  physiologic  responses 
which  are  activated  by  sociopsychologic  pressures.  Whether  or  not  these 
relationships  hold  for  all  species  remains  to  be  determined. 

C.  Natural  Populations 

Evidence  gathered  in  the  laboratory  relating  physiologic  adaptive  mecha- 
nisms to  changes  in  population  density  cannot  prove  that  these  responses 
occur  in  natural  populations.  Even  for  freely  growing  confined  populations 
in  the  laboratory,  the  environmental  conditions  are  quite  altered  by  the 
removal  of  the  effects  of  climatic  and  seasonal  variability  from  those  found 
in  natural  situations.  Preventing  egress  is  another  artificially  imposed 
condition;  although  it  seems  more  likely  that  confinement  parallels  the 
situation  when  populations  reach  densities  where  egress  into  neighboring 
areas  becomes  possible.  Clearly,  evidence  from  natural  populations  is  neces- 
sary before  any  conclusions  are  justified  regarding  the  role  of  physiologic 
adaptive  mechanisms  in  the  regulation  of  mammalian  populations  under 
natural  conditions.  Conclusive  results  are  much  more  difficult  to  obtain  from 
natural  populations  than  from  confined  populations,  as  might  be  expected, 
due  to  the  complexity  and  variability  of  these  populations  and  techincal 
difficulties  in  collecting  the  required  data.  Adequate  controls  and  proper 
assessment  of  environmental  factors  are  often  severe  obstacles,  and  ob- 
taining samples  of  adequate  size  from  populations  at  very  low  densities  is 
intrinsically  very  difficult.  Nevertheless,  evidence  has  been  obtained  to 
indicate  that  the  density-dependent  physiologic  responses  in  natural  popu- 
lations are  similar  to  those  seen  in  experimental  populations.  Finally,  social 
strife  has  been  implicated  (Kalela,  1957)  as  an  important  factor  in  producing 
the  effects  associated  with  increased  density ;  territorial  mutual  intolerance 
increases  greatly  with  sexual  maturity  in  Clethrionomys  rufocanus,  which 
serves  to  increase  tension  even  with  decreased  density  (Kalela,  1957). 
Therefore  behavioral  factors  again  appear  to  be  unportant. 

1.  Adrenocortical  Activity 

The  available  evidence  relating  adrenocortical  activity  to  changes  in 
population  density  has  been  obtained  largely  from  studies  on  populations 


2.  Endocrines  and  Populations  301 

of  Norway  rats,  both  urban  and  rural,  and  voles  {Microtus  sp.),  although 
suggestive  results  have  been  obtained  for  a  few  other  species. 

Adrenal  weight  in  urban  Norway  rats  of  both  sexes  was  shown  to  be 
related  to  population  "density"  in  a  study  of  21  populations  in  Baltimore, 
Md.  (Christian  and  Davis,  1956).  Each  population  was  confined  to  a  city 
block,  the  latter  acting  as  an  island  (Davis,  1953).  The  capacities  of  the 
blocks  for  rats  varied  considerably,  as  did  the  sizes  of  the  populations  them- 
selves, which  resembled  confined  freely  growing  populations  of  house  mice 
in  this  respect.  Therefore,  the  populations  were  categorized  by  their  posi- 
tion on  their  own  growth  curves  at  each  sampling.  A  hypothetic  growth 
curve  was  divided  into  low  stationary,  low  increasing,  high  increasing,  high 
stationary,  and  decreasing,  the  progression  from  low  increasing  through 
decreasing  being  a  progressive  increase  in  relative  density,  although  the 
"density"  status  of  low  stationary  is  equivocal  (Christian  and  Davis,  1956) . 
Each  time  a  sample  was  collected,  the  population  from  which  it  came  was 
put  in  one  of  these  categories.  "Density"  is  obviously  not  strictly  in  terms 
of  area,  but  in  relation  to  the  carrying  capacity  of  the  block  and  the  social 
characteristics  of  the  population.  As  populations  increased  in  relative 
density  from  low  increasing  to  decreasing,  the  mean  adrenal  weight  was 
increased  with  each  relative  density;  so  that  rats  of  both  sexes  from  de- 
creasing populations  had  19%  heavier  adrenals  than  those  from  low,  in- 
creasing populations.  Thymus  weight  was  reciprocally  related  to  adrenal 
weight  in  female  rats,  but  there  was  no  apparent  relationship  between 
thymus  weight  and  adrenal  weight,  with  respect  to  population  density,  in 
male  rats.  The  weights  of  the  pituitary  glands  of  male  rats  were  positively 
related  to  the  adrenal  weights.  That  is,  changes  in  adrenal  weight  were  re- 
flected by  changes  in  pituitary  weight  in  the  male  rats  (Christian  and  Davis, 
1956) .  However,  the  functional  significance  of  these  changes  in  the  weight 
of  the  pituitary  glands  is  not  known.  The  weights  of  the  pituitaries  of  fe- 
males and  thyroid  glands  of  both  sexes  bore  no  apparent  relationship  to 
population  density.  The  data  from  the  low  stationary  populations  are 
difficult  to  evaluate.  The  adrenals  of  female  rats  were  almost  as  light  as 
those  from  low  increasing  populations,  and  their  thymus  glands  were  the 
heaviest  found  in  any  category.  On  the  other  hand,  the  adrenals  of  male 
rats  were  heavier  than  in  any  other  category.  Low,  stationary  populations 
are  extremely  difficult  to  evaluate,  as  the  actual  numbers  of  rats  are  so 
small  that  even  proportionately  large  changes  are  difficult  to  detect.  How- 
ever, these  difficulties  with  their  inherent  errors  in  properly  assigning 
populations  to  a  category  do  not  explain  the  divergence  in  the  male  and 
female  adrenal  weights,  nor  do  they  alter  the  fact  that  these  populations 
were  at  very  low  levels  of  density.  It  should  be  pointed  out  that  the  de- 
creasing populations  were  at  maximum  "density"  because  they  were  de- 
clining naturally  and  therefore  were  at  or  above  the  environmental  capa- 


302  /.  J.  Christian 

city.  It  should  also  be  emphasized  that  food  was  available  in  excess  of  needs 
for  all  populations  and  from  numerous  sources ;  so  that  food  cannot  be  con- 
sidered to  be  an  imprtant  factor  with  respect  to  these  populations. 

Some  of  the  variability  between  urban  populations  of  rats  may  be  ex- 
plained by  the  fact  that  the  rat  population  in  each  block  is  divided  into 
several  discrete  colonics  when  the  total  population  is  at  a  relatively  low 
density  (at  high  density  these  colonies  coalesce  and  the  population  evidently 
becomes  a  single  unit  throughout  the  block).  The  colonies  comprising 
several  of  the  block-populations  were  evaluated  individually  with  respect 
to  relative  density  in  the  same  way  as  the  populations  for  the  entire  blocks 
(Christian  and  Davis,  1956).  Relative  density  values  were  assigned  and 
compared  with  the  adrenal  weights  of  the  rats  from  these  colonies.  A  sug- 
gestive correlation  between  adrenal  weights  and  relative  population  den- 
sities of  these  colonies  was  found,  although  the  differences  were  not  signifi- 
cant, probably  owing  to  the  small  number  of  samples.  The  sample  size  was 
limited  by  the  relatively  few  colonies  from  which  a  sufficient  number  of  rats 
were  collected  for  comparative  purposes. 

Although  adrenal  weight  is  greatest  in  naturally  declining  populations 
(Christian  and  Davis,  1956) ,  it  was  shown  in  another  study  that  artificially 
reducing  populations  of  rats  produces  an  immediate  and  proportional  reduc- 
tion in  adrenal  weight  (Christian  and  Davis,  1955).  Adrenal  weight  in  both 
sexes  of  rats  from  three  populations  was  reduced  38%  by  intensive  trapping. 
The  adrenal  weights  were  maintained  at  this  level  for  5  months  by  main- 
taining the  populations  at  the  reduced  level. 

In  another  study  a  rural  population  of  Norway  rats  was  followed  by 
monthly  sampling  for  two  years  (Christian,  1959b).  An  index  of  the  size 
of  the  population  was  obtained  each  time  a  sample  was  collected  by  using 
a  standardized  trapping  procedure.  The  weights  of  the  adrenals,  pituitaries, 
and  thyroids  were  determined  for  each  sample.  There  was  a  highly  signifi- 
cant correlation  between  adrenal  weight  and  population  index  for  both 
sexes,  and  an  even  more  significant  correlation  between  pituitary  and 
adrenal  weight  for  both  sexes  for  the  24  monthly  samples  in  the  two-year 
period.  The  functional  significance  of  the  pituitary  changes  is  not  known; 
it  can  only  be  inferred  from  data  on  weight.  Evidently  changes  in  the  rate 
of  ACTH  secretion,  as  indicated  by  adrenal  weight,  are  accompanied  by 
parallel  changes  in  pituitary  weight.  Changes  in  thyroid  weight  and  in 
thymus  weight  were  not  correlated  with  changes  in  the  population  size, 
adrenal  weight,  or  pituitary  weight. 

The  results  of  the  foregoing  experiments  indicate  that  adrenocortical 
activity  in  Norway  rats  from  natural  urban  or  rural  populations  is  related 
directly  to  changes  in  population  density  and  that  both  sexes  respond  in  the 
same  way.  Therefore  it  appears  that  physiologic  adaptive  mechanisms  are 


2.  Endocrines  and  Populations  303 

operative  in  natural  populations  of  Norway  rats,  just  as  they  are  in  freely 
growing  confined  populations  of  house  mice  or  voles. 

Another  series  of  experiments  with  populations  of  rats  produced  results 
which  at  first  glance  appeared  to  contradict  the  foregoing  conclusions.  Alien 
rats  were  introduced  into  stationary  and  increasing  populations  to  verify 
earlier  observations  that  such  a  procedure  profoundly  affected  the  popula- 
tion (Calhoun,  1948).  When  stationary  populations  of  rats  were  increased 
20%  by  adding  alien  rats  of  one  sex  or  the  other,  the  populations  declined 
abruptly  to  about  three-fifths  their  original  values.  When  large  numbers  of 
rats  were  substituted  (native  rats  were  removed  and  replaced  by  an  equal 
number  of  aliens)  into  increasing  populations,  the  populations  promptly 
ceased  growing.  These  procedures  presumably  produced  severe  social  strife 
with  a  marked  increase  in  mortality  (Calhoun,  1948;  Davis,  1953).  How- 
ever, the  adrenal  weights  of  rats  taken  at  regular  sampling  intervals  failed 
to  reflect  the  increased  strife  and  closely  followed  the  course  of  the  popula- 
tion size.  The  explanation  of  this  apparent  paradox  is  clear.  The  introduced 
rats  were,  of  course,  aliens — hence  subordinate  rats — in  a  strange  environ- 
ment (see  Clarke,  1953;  Barnett,  1955,  1958)  and  therefore  became  mor- 
tality statistics.  The  native  rats  which  succumbed  were  probably  subordi- 
nate animals  in  the  original  population.  The  adrenal  weights  were  only  from 
survivors  and  therefore  probably  from  dominant  animals.  It  already  has 
been  pointed  out  that  adrenal  weight  is  least  in  dominant  animals,  and  so 
the  increase  in  adrenal  weight  which  might  have  been  anticipated  from  a 
superficial  examination  of  the  situation  obviously  and  simply  failed  to 
appear.  Had  the  adrenals  of  the  rats  that  died  been  obtainable,  they  un- 
questionably would  have  exhibited  profound  changes. 

Natural  populations  of  voles  (Microtus)  have  also  been  studied  to  deter- 
mine the  relation  between  adrenal  function  and  population  density.  Adams, 
Bell,  and  Moore  (Christian,  1959b)  periodically  collected  frequent  samples 
from  a  natural  population  of  Microtus  montanus  in  Montana  for  four  years 
to  obtain  indices  of  population  density,  adrenal  weights,  and  data  on  re- 
productive activity.  The  number  of  males  collected  was  inadequate  for 
valid  comparisons.  However,  population  density  and  the  adrenal  weights 
of  female  voles  were  closely  correlated.  The  population  was  at  peak  densities 
in  late  summer  of  1952  after  which  there  was  a  continued  general  overall 
decline  in  the  population  until  the  summer  of  1955.  The  adrenal  weights  of 
females  were  maximum  in  the  late  summer  of  1952  and  then  underwent  a 
gradual  overall  decline,  generally  following  the  population.  However,  within 
each  year  there  were  marked  seasonal  changes  in  the  population  which  were 
reflected  by  equally  marked  changes  in  adrenal  weights.  The  population 
reached  its  annual  maximum  density  in  late  summer  or  early  fall  and  then 
declined  sharply  to  very  low  levels  where  it  remained  until  late  winter  or 


304  /.  ,/.  Christian 

early  spring  when  breeding  recommenced  and  the  populations  increased 
sharply.  Changes  in  adrenal  weight  closely  followed  these  animal  cycles  in 
population  density.  Peak  adrenal  weights  were  found  in  early  fall  and  coin- 
cided with  a  cessation  of  reproduction  and  the  beginning  of  the  autumnal 
decline  in  the  populations.  Adrenal  weights  were  minimal  by  November 
and  remained  there  until  March, ^  when  the  population  again  began  to  in- 
crease. Adrenal  weights  were  maximum  in  the  last  summer  of  1952,  aver- 
aging about  15%  greater  than  in  any  other  year  for  a  comparable  time  of 
year.  The  mean  weights  in  winter  were  roughly  60%  less  than  the  maxi- 
mum. These  results  indicate  that  the  supposedly  severe  winter  conditions  of 
the  Rocky  Mountain  Region  of  Northern  Montana  do  not  impose  physio- 
logic hardships  on  voles.  Climatic  conditions,  if  they  affected  the  animals 
at  all,  apparently  were  unimportant  compared  to  changes  in  population 
density.  The  sharp  declines  in  the  population  every  fall  evidently  were  due 
to  a  cessation  of  breeding  and  continuing  random  mortality,  not  to  a  de- 
clining environment,  otherwise  the  adrenal  weights  would  not  have  de- 
creased so  spectacularly  (Christian,  1959b).  It  is  possible  that  physiologic 
responses  to  high  densities  in  late  summer  contributed  to  the  annual  de- 
clines in  population  density,  but  it  seems  unlikely  except  in  1952,  In  that 
year  it  is  probable  that  physiologic  adaptive  responses  were  at  least  partly 
responsible  for  the  decrease  in  the  size  of  the  population.  It  is  noteworthy 
that  the  rapid  decline  from  peak  densities  in  the  late  summer  and  early 
fall  of  1952  was  not  caused  by  increased  mortality  from  disease  (Adams  et 
al.;  cited  in  Christian,  1959b). 

Adrenocortical-density  relationships  were  studied  by  Louch  (1958)  in 
two  natural  populations  of  voles  (Microtus  pcnnsylvanicus) .  He  used 
eosinophil  counts,  supplemented  by  adrenal  weights,  as  indices  of  adreno- 
cortical activity.  Relative  adrenal  weights  were  high  and  eosinophil  counts 
remarkably  low  in  one  population  at  peak  density.  Following  a  sharp  de- 
cline in  this  population,  indicated  by  an  85%  drop  in  the  population  index, 
the  eosinophil  counts  rose  sharply  about  500%  and  continued  to  rise  more 
gradually  for  the  subsequent  nine  months  of  the  study.  Adrenal  weight 
averaged  59%  lower  during  the  period  of  low  density  than  during  the  pre- 
ceding period  of  high  density:  14.5  mg./lOO  gm.  compared  to  23.0  mg./lOO 
gm.  The  size  of  the  second  population  remained  low  tliroughout  the  experi- 
mental period.  The  eosinophil  counts  began  high  and  gradually  rose  to 
higher  levels.  Adrenal  weight  declined  about  27%  (from  a  mean  of  19.4  to 
a  mean  of  15.2  mg./lOO  gm.)  in  the  second  population  over  the  same  period 

3  Subsequent  work  has  suggested  that  the  low  winter  adrenal  weights  are  partly  due 
to  sexual  inactivity  and  regression,  although  not  entirely  as  a  number  of  the  voles  were 
not  sexually  regressed.  In  other  words  the  adrenal  weights  should  be  low  due  to  density 
factors  (cf.  October),  but  these  were  too  low  [see  Christian  (1961,  1963b)l. 


2.  Endocrincs  and  Populations  305 

as  the  59%  decline  in  the  first  population.  However,  the  decline  was  not 
nearly  so  great  and  the  initial  weight  was  19%  less  than  in  the  first  popula- 
tion. The  lower  adrenal  weight  was  reflected  by  a  higher  mean  eosinophil 
for  the  same  period  of  time.  Even  though  the  reason  for  the  lower  adrenal 
weight  in  the  second  population  during  the  second  period  is  not  known, 
adrenal  weight  and  eosinophil  counts  in  these  studies  generally  reflected 
differences  in  population  density  (Louch,  1958). 

Methods  for  estimating  most  natural  populations  of  mammals  are  at 
best  relatively  crude,  insensitive,  and  subject  to  many  errors.  At  best  they 
can  detect  only  relatively  large  changes  in  population  density.  The  catch 
per  trap-night  or  any  comparable  index  of  population  density  obtained  by 
some  sort  of  uniform  trapping  effort  is  probably  as  good  an  index  as  any 
readily  usable  procedure.  The  catch  per  unit  of  time  was  used  in  these 
studies  of  vole  populations  by  Louch  (1958)  and  Adams  et  al.  (Christian, 
1959b) ,  as  well  as  in  the  studies  of  rural  populations  of  Norway  rats  (Chris- 
tian, 1959b) .  Errors  in  estimating  population  density  in  this  fashion  may 
account  for  most  of  the  observed  discrepancies.  If  the  existence  of  physio- 
logic responses  to  population  density  is  established  for  a  variety  of  species, 
it  is  likely  that  the  magnitude  of  these  responses  will  provide  a  much  more 
precise  index  of  relative  population  density  (the  important  figure  for  practi- 
cal purposes)  than  any  existing  indirect  method  for  determining  population 
size.^ 

Limited  studies  with  natural  populations  of  other  species  have  been  made. 
A  68%  decline  in  the  index  of  the  size  of  a  population  of  white-footed  mice 
{Peromyscus  leucopus)  from  one  July  to  the  next  was  accompanied  by  a 
58%  decline  in  their  mean  adrenal  weight  (Christian,  1959b).  Similar,  but 
less  conclusive,  results  have  been  obtained  with  natural  populations  of 
short-tailed  shrews  [Blarina)  (Christian,  1954,  and  unpublished).  Beer 
and  Meyer  (1951)  studied  the  seasonal  changes  in  the  endocrine  and 
i-eproductive  organs  of  muskrats  and  found  a  marked  peak  in  adrenal 
weight  in  adults  of  both  sexes  in  early  fall  and  a  second  minor  peak  in 
March  and  April,  especially  in  adult  females.  These  seasonal  changes  are 
similar  to  those  we  have  noted  in  Microtus  montanus  and  may  be  related 
to  behavioral  and  social  changes. 

Preliminary  work  on  the  relationships  of  the  weights  of  deer  adrenals  to 

"  The  persistence  of  immature  zonation  in  the  adrenals  of  males  and  lack  of  hyper- 
trophy due  estrogenic  stimulation  in  females  may  explain  some  of  the  discrepant  re- 
sults with  Microtus,  and  possibty  Lemmus,  as  these  would  seriously  confound  adrenal 
weight  relationships  if  the  population  under  study  is  composed  of  an  appreciable  per- 
centage of  sexuallj'  immature,  regressed,  or  otherwise  sexually  inactive  animals.  Winter 
or  high-density  populations  will  consist  largely  of  such  animals  [see  Christian  (19(il, 
1963b)]. 


306  J-  J-  Christian 

various  factors  indicates  that  adrenocortical  physiology  in  this  species,  as 
in  others,  reflects  environmental  changes,  and  the  adrenal  was  therefore 
said  to  be  an  indicator  of  conditions  as  in  any  other  species.  (Hughes  and 
Mall,  1958).  However,  sika  deer  (Cervus  nippon)  respond  to  changes  in 
population  density  in  a  manner  quite  similar  to  that  seen  in  rats  (Christian 
et  al.,  1960) .  A  60%  decline  in  the  population  of  these  deer  was  accompanied 
by  a  proportional  decline  in  adrenal  weight.  The  decline  of  this  population 
was  brought  about  by  a  mass  mortality  which  apparently  was  due  to 
metabolic  disturbances  resulting  from  the  prolonged  adrenocortical  hyper- 
activity associated  with  a  high  density  of  the  population.  There  is  evidence 
that  potassium  deficiency  resulting  from  cortical  hyperactivity  may  have 
contributed  directly  to  the  mortality.  Marked  stunting  of  growth  was  also 
seen  during  the  period  of  high  density  and  especially  during  the  year  of 
die-off.  Other  possible  causes  of  the  die-off,  such  as  malnutrition,  could  be 
ruled  out. 

More  experimental  work  is  needed  to  relate  adrenal  function  with  popula- 
tion density  for  a  number  of  species  in  natural  populations.  The  problems 
are  numerous  and  not  the  least  of  these  is  being  able  to  rule  out  extraneous 
factors  or  else  to  assess  their  role  in  producing  the  measured  effects. 

2.  Reproductive  Function 

A  large  number  of  studies  suggest  that  reproductive  function  is  depressed 
with  increasing  density  of  natural  populations,  but  studies  sufficiently 
discriminative  to  attribute  changes  in  reproductive  function  to  changes 
in  density  per  se  without  additional  complications  are  few.  Therefore  a 
great  deal  of  the  evidence  is  circumstantial  and  tentative  at  best.  The 
present  discussion  is  limited  to  those  studies  which  have  been  conducted 
with  suflficient  care  and  attention  to  a  variety  of  details  so  that  one  is 
confident  that  the  factors  have  been  properly  considered  which  possibly 
might  affect  reproduction.  However,  a  brief  discussion  of  various  problems 
involved  in  evaluating  reproductive  function  is  in  order  before  considering 
the  evidence  implicating  physiologic  responses  to  population  density  in 
the  suppression  of  reproductive  function. 

First  there  is  the  problem  of  the  food  supply  of  the  experimental  popula- 
tion. As  we  have  seen,  there  is  ample  evidence  that  food  deficiencies  can 
curtail  reproductive  function  independently  of  other  adaptive  mechanisms. 
However,  the  effects  of  food  and  of  increased  density  on  the  reproductive 
system  and  growth  are  so  similar  that  unless  careful  assessments  are  made 
of  the  food  supply  (preferably  these  should  be  accompanied  by  an  evalua- 
tion of  adrenocortical  and  other  adaptive  fvmctions,  no  separation  can  be 
made  between  the  effects  of  food  and  of  density,  especially  at  relatively 


2.  Endocrines  and  Populations  307 

high  population  densities.  Furthermore,  it  is  very  likely  that  the  adaptive 
reactions  to  density  and  their  effects  may  overlap  food  shortages,  so  that 
their  effects  are  mutually  augmentive  at  critical  densities.  However,  the 
aim  of  the  present  discussion  is  to  assemble  the  evidence  that  adaptive 
reactions  to  density  occur  irrespective  of  whether  or  not  the  effects  of  in- 
adequate food  or  other  environmental  factors  are  superimposed.  The  growth 
of  populations  unquestionably  can  be  limited  by  environmental  factors 
which  may  either  act  through  the  physiologic  adaptive  mechanisms  or  in- 
dependently of  them,  especially  in  localized  populations.  However,  the 
basic  concept  of  physiologic  adaptation  to  population  density  is  that  these 
mechanisms  are  always  operative,  and  will  regulate  and  can  limit  popula- 
tion growth.  Finally,  limitations  of  enviroimiental  factors,  even  of  food, 
may  increase  competition  directly  and  therefore  indirectly  produce  in- 
creased activity  of  the  physiologic  adaptive  mechanisms.  Pitelka's  (1957b) 
statement  that  "the  interest  in  the  stress  mechanisms  has  led  some  students 
to  overlook  the  point  that  such  a  mechanism  does  not  evolve  without 
linkages  to  critical  variables  extrinsic  to  the  population"  bear  repeating 
and  is  emphasized  and  supported  by  an  immense  amount  of  work  with  rat 
populations  (Davis,  1953).  Nevertheless,  it  is  extremely  miportant  to  dis- 
tinguish between  the  direct  action  of  environmental  variables  on  popula- 
tions and  their  indirect  action  through  sociopsychologic  and  physiologic 
mechanisms;  and  experimental  evidence  to  date  emphasizes  the  general 
importance  of  the  latter  mode  of  action.  Evidence  of  the  direct  action  of 
food  shortages  in  depressing  reproduction  in  natural  populations  of  Pero- 
myscus  boylii  and  P.  maniculatus  is  cited  by  Jameson  (1953,  1955),  and 
population  declines  evidently  followed  the  shortages  as  a  result  of  con- 
tinuing normal  mortality  in  the  presence  of  a  lull  in  reproductive  activity 
with  no  recruitment  into  the  populations.  The  evidence  presented  by  Jame- 
son, supported  by  the  controlled  experiments  previously  described,  strongly 
favors  the  interpretation  that  inadequate  food  supplies  were  directly  re- 
sponsible for  the  inhibition  of  reproduction.  However,  the  possibility  that 
the  shortages  induced  increased  competition  and  increased  activity  of 
adaptive  responses  were  not  ruled  out  completely.  Davis  (1951c),  in  a 
study  of  Norway  rats  from  natural  populations,  provided  definitive  evi- 
dence that  a  deficient  food  supply  can  inhibit  reproduction  under  natural 
circumstances.  However,  w^hether  the  effects  were  direct  or  produced  by 
increasing  competition  with  stimulation  of  generalized  physiologic  adaptive 
response  is  unknown.  It  was  determined  later  that  the  adrenals  of  these 
rats  were  responsive  to  changes  in  population  density  (Christian,  1959b). 
Other  examples  could  be  cited,  but  the  majority  could  serve  only  to  empha- 
size the  fact  that  discrimination  between  the  effects  of  food  and  other  factors 
is  usually  not  attempted. 


308  /.  J.  Christian 

Another,  perhaps  more  serious  problem  in  evaluating  the  effects  of 
density  on  reproduction  is  the  lack  of  adequate  criteria  to  determine  the 
age  of  most  small  mammals;  so  that  weight  and  length  measurements  are 
usually  used  as  criteria  of  age.  Since  one  of  the  major  effects  of  increased 
activity  of  physiologic  adaptive  responses  is  diminished  growth,  as  well  as 
inhibition  of  reproductive  function,  there  is  no  valid  way  to  separate  normal 
immature  animals  from  older  animals  which  should  be  mature  but  in  which 
both  growth  and  reproductive  maturity  are  inhibited.  Several  serious 
misinterpretations  can  result  from  this  situation :  ( 1 )  a  shift  of  age  composi- 
tion of  the  population  toward  greater  age  will  be  obscured;  and  (2)  repro- 
ductive competence  is  usually  assessed  on  the  basis  of  the  prevalence  of 
pregnancy,  prevalence  of  lactation,  and  similar  criteria  in  those  animals 
which  are  obviously  mature,  therefore  total  suppression  of  reproductive 
activity  with  a  delay  in  the  onset  of  puberty  will  be  missed  altogether.  Of 
course  in  such  a  situation,  the  reproductively  active  and  obviously  mature 
animals  represent  only  the  portion  of  the  population  which  is  least  affected 
by  density  factors,  a  situation  to  which  attention  has  been  called  earlier. 
The  studies  of  Crowcroft  and  Rowe  (1957)  offer  a  clear  illustration  of 
this  situation  in  controlled  populations  in  which  the  ages  of  the  animals 
were  known.  How  would  the  nonreproductive  mice  in  these  populations 
have  been  classified  in  the  usual  studies  of  natural  populations  with  trapped 
animals?  Kalela's  (1957)  studies  on  the  effects  of  population  density  on 
Clethrionomys  rufocanus  from  a  natural  population  are  of  singular  value 
because  (1)  he  had  a  means  of  determining  the  age  of  the  animals  by  the 
rooting  of  their  molars  and  was  therefore  able  to  state  with  certainty  that 
growth  and  maturity  were  totally  inhibited,  and  (2)  he  eliminated  the 
possibility  that  food  was  an  etiologic  agent  in  producing  these  phenomena. 
Of  course,  all  gradations  of  these  effects  may  occur  to  further  confuse  the 
situation.  Most  studies  which  evaluate  changes  in  reproductive  function  of 
small  mammals,  for  which  there  is  no  adequate  way  of  determining  age,  in 
relation  to  density  in  populations  must  be  viewed  with  considerable  skepti- 
cism. It  has  already  been  pointed  out  that  the  usual  means  of  determining 
reproductive  competence  do  not  detect  changes  short  of  almost  total 
inhibition,  especially  in  males. 

A  third  problem,  less  frequent  in  its  occurrence,  is  the  failure  to  take  into 
consideration  changes  in  age  composition  when  evaluating  changes  in  litter 
size,  although  this  frequently  results  from  the  inability  to  determine  age. 
Hamilton  (1937)  reported  that  litter  size  in  Microtus  pennsijlvanicus  in- 
creased with  increasing  population  density  and  yet  there  was  increased 
intra-uterine  mortality  at  the  higher  densities.  It  is  well  known  that  litter 
size  increases  with  parity  for  the  first  few  litters  and  that  there  usually  is  a 
direct  relationship  between  body  weight  and  litter  size  for  most  litter- 


2.  Endocriues  and  Populations  309 

bearing  mammals  (Watt,  1934;  Hoffman,  1957;  Flick  et  at.,  1959).  There- 
fore it  is  entirely  possible  that  the  apparent  increase  in  litter  size  noted  by- 
Hamilton  was  a  result  of  a  shift  in  age  composition,  and  even  if  this  is  not 
the  case,  the  point  is  generally  valid  and  is  well  illustrated  by  Hamilton's 
data.  These  data  bring  up  another  point  which  often  proves  confusing; 
that  is  there  is  no  word  in  use  to  designate  an  in  utero  group  of  embryos. 
"Litter"  used  for  both  postpartum  and  antepartum  young  with  the  result 
that  ''litter  size"  is  frequently  determined  from  embryo  counts  and  conclu- 
sions drawn  therefrom  regarding  litter  size  at  birth.  Although  the  authors 
are  themselves  usually  quite  clear  on  these  differences,  their  descriptions  in 
the  literature  often  are  not,  and  the  reader  will  arrive  at  false  conclusions 
or  else  be  unable  to  make  any  at  all.  This  situation  can  be  corrected  by  the 
use  of  the  collective  noiui  gravidum  to  describe  the  in  utero  counterpart  of 
"htter"  (Snyder  and  Christian,  1960). 

Finally,  there  is  a  wide  divergence  in  the  descriptions  of  the  effects  of 
population  density  on  reproduction.  It  seems  clear  from  experimental  and 
field  data  that  the  reproductive  processes  can  be  inhibited  at  a  number  of 
different  points,  evidently  depending  on  the  severity  of  the  stimulus,  dura- 
tion of  the  stimulus,  age  at  which  the  stimulus  was  first  effective,  the  posi- 
tion of  any  given  animal  or  groups  of  animals  in  the  social  structure,  prob- 
ably individual  physiologic  and  psychologic  differences,  as  well  as  other 
unknown  factors.  The  details  of  these  various  relationships  are  not  known, 
but  experimental  results  suggest  that  the  smallest  stimulus  inducing  an  in- 
creased adaptive  response  with  inhibition  of  reproduction  in  mature  ani- 
mals will  affect  lactation,  increase  litter  mortality,  and  possibly  depress 
fertility;  a  greater  stimulus  will  result  in  increased  intra-uterine  mortality 
and  an  even  greater  stimulus  will  totally  suppress  reproductive  function. 
It  is  suggested  that  as  the  alarming  stimulus  increases  in  severity  there  is 
a  progressively  greater  suppression  of  the  secretion  of  gonadotropins.  It  is 
equally  evident  that  young  mammals  reaching  maturity  are  far  more  sus- 
ceptible to  a  given  alarming  stimulus  than  an  animal  which  has  already 
reached  maturity.  It  is  known  that  immature  rats  and  mice  are  many 
times  more  sensitive  to  suppression  of  gonadotropins  by  steroids  than  the 
mature  animals  (Byrnes  and  Meyer,  1951).  The  results  of  experiments 
discussed  in  the  preceding  sections  show  that  these  statements  apply  to  the 
effects  of  population  density.  The  effects  of  vitamin  and  other  nutritional 
deficiencies  follow  a  similar  pattern;  furthermore,  the  earlier  in  the  process 
of  reproduction  the  deficiency  begins,  the  more  profound  is  its  effect  on  the 
developing  fetuses  (Lutwak-Mann,  1958).  Usually  a  deficiency  beginning 
at  the  start  of  pregnancy  or  earlier  will  produce  a  very  high  percentage  of 
resorptions.  These  decrease  the  later  the  deficiency  begins.  It  is  noteworthy 
that  only  "borderline"  deficiencies  appear  to  produce  congenital  anomalies. 


310  J.  J.  Christian 

So  it  appears  to  be  with  population  density.  Only  those  female  mice  which 
were  moderately  affected  by  density  produced  young  which  were  then 
permanently  affected  by  reduction  in  lactation,  whereas  those  mice  more 
seriously  affected  failed  to  produce  at  all  and  exhibited  100%  resorption  of 
embryos  (Christian  and  LeMunyan,  1958).  Therefore,  when  one  considers 
the  enormous  number  of  variables  affecting  a  population,  it  is  not  sur- 
prising that  there  is  so  much  variation  among  individuals  and  among 
populations  wdth  respect  to  the  effects  on  reproduction.  Reproduction 
should  include  the  entire  process  of  producing  young  from  maturation  of  the 
parents  to  weaning  when  discussing  or  measuring  the  effects  of  physiologic 
adaptive  mechanisms  and  the  effects  on  the  endocrine  organs. 

In  spite  of  these  problems,  a  number  of  studies  on  natural  populations  of 
small  mammals  have  been  sufficientlj''  detailed  and  critical  to  permit  exami- 
nation of  the  relationships  between  population  density  and  reproduction. 
The  most  conclusive  of  these  have  been  carried  out  on  Norway  rats  (Rattus 
norvegicus)  and  voles  {Microtus  sp.  and  Clethrionoimjs) ,  but  there  have  been 
others  on  muskrats  (Ondatra),  cotton  rats  {Sigtnodon) ,  woodchucks  {Mar- 
mota) ,  and  hares  (Lepus  americanus) .  Most  of  these  suffer  to  one  extent 
or  another  from  a  lack  of  dependable  criteria  for  determining  age,  as  dis- 
cussed above. 

The  reproducti^'e  performance  of  Norway  rats  from  increasing,  de- 
creasing, and  stationary  populations  was  investigated  (Davis,  1951a). 
These  rats  have  a  major  reproductive  season  in  late  winter  and  early 
spring  and  a  secondary  minor  peak  in  reproductive  activity  in  the  fall. 
Increasing  populations  have  a  higher  prevalence  of  pregnancy  than  either 
decreasing  or  stationary  populations.  These  differences  are  especially 
marked  in  the  spring  breeding  season  when  the  prevalence  of  pregnancies 
was  41.6%  in  increasing,  25.3%  in  decreasing,  and  14.4%  in  stationary 
populations.  The  incidences  of  pregnancy  were,  repsectively,  6.1,  6.3,  and 
3.8  per  year.  However,  there  were  no  apparent  differences  in  litter  size  and 
the  prevalence  of  lactation  Avas  essentially  the  same  in  all  three  categories 
although  the  data  on  lactation  could  not  be  analyzed  separately  for  the 
two  breeding  seasons.  However,  an  analysis  of  lactation  by  seasons  could 
not  account  for  the  similarity  of  the  overall  figure  and  one  must  conclude 
paradoxically  that,  although  the  pregnancy  rate  was  highest  in  increasing 
populations,  there  was  also  a  greater  parturitional  mortality  and  at  the 
same  time  a  better  survival  of  those  young  which  were  not  lost  soon  after 
birth. 

Similar  results  were  obtained  when  reproductive  performances  of  Norway 
rats  from  a  rural  population  were  compared  at  two  different  relative  popula- 
tion densities.  The  pregnancy  rate  was  48.3%  a  month  after  artificially 
reducing  the  population  a  third,  compared  to  14.4%  before  the  reduction. 


2.  Endocrines  and  Populations  311 

These  results  indicate  that  reproductive  performance  is  inversely  related 
to  population  density  in  Norway  rats,  at  least  for  increasing  and  stationary 
populations.  The  status  of  decreasing  populations  is  equivocal  and  may 
represent  a  partial  recovery  of  reproductive  function  in  declining  popula- 
tions, although  decreasing  populations  are  presumably  at  maximum  den- 
sity. However,  we  already  have  noted  that  adrenocortical  hypertrophy  is 
greatest  in  populations  spontaneously  decreasing  from  asymptotic  levels. 
These  results  represent  a  large  number  of  populations  and  carry  consider- 
able weight;  nevertheless  in  another  experiment  in  which  three  urban 
populations  of  Norway  rats  were  artificially  reduced  about  50%  there  was 
no  corresponding  increase  in  the  rates  of  pregnancy  (Davis  and  Christian, 
1958).  Two  factors  may  have  obscured  such  an  occurrence.  One  was  that 
the  populations  were  increasing  and  therefore  had  a  fairly  high  pregnancy 
rate  at  the  time  of  the  reduction  (30.2%) ,  and  the  other  was  that  the  main 
breeding  season  was  declining  at  the  time  the  next  samples  were  collected 
(April,  May,  and  June)  from  which  a  mean  pregnancy  rate  of  32.2%  was 
obtained. 

Experiments  designed  to  analyze  reproductive  function  in  relation  to 
density  of  populations  of  rats  suffer  from  the  inability  to  determine  the 
age  of  the  animals,  therefore  attainment  of  maturity  and  other  reproduc- 
tive end  points  are  based  on  the  sizes  of  the  animals.  There  is  evidence  to 
indicate  that  rats  in  increasing  populations  grow  faster,  and  so  are  larger 
at  a  given  age,  than  rats  from  stationary  populations  (Davis,  1951b). 
Therefore  inhibition  of  growth  in  populations  of  high  density  may  tend  to 
obscure  evidence  of  partial  inhibition  of  reproductive  function,  such  as 
delayed  puberty,  since  it  is  likely  that  reproductive  function  and  growth 
are  equally  inhibited.  Thus,  these  results  are  evidence  to  support  the  earlier 
comments  regarding  the  desirability  of  determining  age  in  mammals  when 
evaluating  reproductive  function. 

One  of  the  more  conclusive  studies  to  date  on  the  relationship  between 
density  of  population  and  reproduction  was  conducted  by  Kalela  (1957) 
on  red-backed  voles  {Clethrionomijs  rufocanus)  in  Finland.  A  major  factor 
contributing  to  the  value  of  this  study  was  the  ability  to  determine  and 
approximate  age  of  the  animals  by  the  root  development  of  the  first  molars. 
These  molars  are  rootless  at  the  end  of  September  of  the  year  of  birth,  but 
the  roots  are  visible  by  the  following  spring  and  continue  to  develop  further 
(Zimmermann,  1937;  Kalela,  1957).  The  question  might  be  raised  whether 
the  growth  and  rooting  of  the  molars  would  be  suppressed  along  with  sup- 
pression of  growth,  but  molars  were  rooted  in  animals  in  which  growth  and 
maturity  were  definitely  suppressed  (Kalela,  1957) ;  so  that  any  suppression 
of  molar  growi^h  and  rooting  would  make  the  results  even  more  dramatic. 
[]This  and  other  means  of  estimating  age  in  voles  has  been  more  fully  re- 


312  /.  /.  Christian 

viewed  by  Bourliere  (1951).]  Kalela  also  determined  that  mature  sperma- 
tozoa are  present  in  the  testes  of  these  voles  when  the  testes  are  seven  or 
more  millimeters  long  and  defined  maturity  on  this  basis.  Females  which 
were  pregnant  or  parous  were  defined  as  fecund. 

During  the  first  summer  of  Kalela's  studies  in  1954,  moderate  population 
densities  were  achieved  after  a  spring  characterized  by  a  very  small  popula- 
tion which  had  overwintered  from  the  preceding  fall.  Males  and  females 
from  the  litters  born  early  in  the  breeding  season  matured  without  excep- 
tion. Four-fifths  of  the  mature  females  of  late  summer  were  young  of  the 
year.  A  large  number  of  the  young  of  the  year  which  reached  maturity  had 
two  litters  and  some  even  had  thi-ee  during  their  first  summer.  The  old 
overwintering  females  had  as  many  as  four  litters.  The  following  spring, 
1955,  the  population  started  with  a  much  larger  number  of  overwintering 
animals.  The  rate  of  growth  of  this  population  at  first  was  as  high  or  higher 
than  the  rate  of  growth  of  the  population  in  the  preceding  year,  but  it 
rapidly  declined  as  the  density  of  the  population  increased.  Peak  densities 
were  reached  in  this  year.  A  good  many  of  the  males  born  in  the  first  part 
of  the  breeding  season  reached  maturity,  but  males  with  enlarged  testes 
were  rarely  found  in  August,  and  there  was  an  accumulation  of  immature 
males  weighing  between  20  and  24  gm.  at  this  time  (mature  red-backed 
voles  normally  weigh  more  than  24  gm.,  usually  between  30  and  40  gm.). 
This  weight  range  represents  the  maximum  size  achieved  by  male  or  fe- 
male red-backed  voles  which  fail  to  mature,  irrespective  of  their  age.  There 
was  no  accumulation  of  voles  in  this  weight  category  during  the  preceding 
year  of  low  population  densities.  Female  young  of  the  year  exhibited  the 
same  cessation  of  growth  and  failure  to  mature,  except  for  those  born  early 
in  the  breeding  season.  Less  than  half  of  the  mature  females  in  late  summer 
were  young  of  the  year  and  there  was  a  marked  accumulation  of  immature 
females  in  the  20-24  gm.  M^eight  class.  Furthermore,  the  pregnancy  rate  fell 
sharply  and  had  fallen  to  well  below  the  1954  levels  by  mid-August.  The 
overwintering  females  had  up  to  three  litters,  but  none  had  four  litters. 
Those  young  of  the  year  which  reached  maturity  had  no  more  than  two 
litters  and  only  a  few  had  this  many.  One  area  (Malla)  had  twice  the 
density  of  the  others  under  study  at  this  time,  and  none  of  the  young  of  the 
year,  male  or  female,  attained  sexual  maturity  or  grew  beyond  the  20-24 
gm.  category. 

The  population  had  undergone  a  major  decline  in  numbers  by  the  spring 
of  1956  and  was  well  below  the  1954  levels  for  the  same  period,  and  yet  the 
rate  of  growth  of  the  population  was  extremely  slow.  The  overwintering 
animals,  as  well  as  the  young  of  the  year,  were  distinctly  smaller  than 
normal.  Reproductive  performance  was  essentially  identical  to  that  of  the 
preceding  summer  of  1955  when  the  density  of  the  population  was  extreme. 


2.  Endocrines  and  Populations  313 

The  rates  of  fecundity,  pregnancy,  and  other  measurements  of  reproductive 
competence  were  unchanged  from  the  preceding  year.  There  was  no  appar- 
ent change  in  htter  size  or  intra-uterine  mortaUty  throughout  the  period  of 
the  study  of  this  population.  There  was  no  shortage  of  food  at  any  time 
during  these  studies,  and  especially  not  during  periods  of  active  reproduc- 
tion. Therefore,  the  changes  in  reproductive  function  are  not  attributable 
to  ^'arying  food  supplies.  Climatic  factors,  although  variable,  did  not  corre- 
late with  the  functional  changes  and  were  probably  noncontributory  in 
producing  functional  changes. 

This  study  shows,  beyond  reasonable  doubt,  that  the  shifts  in  reproduc- 
tive function  were  reciprocally  related  to  the  density  of  the  population  in 
1954  and  1955  and  that  increased  density  was  the  factor  responsible  for 
the  decline  in  reproductive  function  (with  a  total  suppression  of  reproduc- 
tion in  the  young  of  the  year  in  one  area)  and  attainment  of  maturity  in 
1955.  It  also  shows  that  the  primary  effect  of  increased  density  was  to  in- 
hibit maturation  so  that  large  numbers  of  immature  animals  accumulated 
by  late  summer.  At  the  same  time,  growth  beyond  a  general  level  of  20-24 
gm.  was  suppressed  in  both  sexes.  The  data  from  the  area  of  extreme 
density,  in  which  no  animals  matured  are  especially  convincing.  No  direct 
measures  of  endocrine  function  were  made,  but  these  occurrences  in  natural 
populations  of  Clethrionomys  rufocanus  conform  with  the  results  from  the 
more  highly  controlled  experiments  with  populations  of  mice  or  voles  in 
the  laboratory. 

The  apparently  paradoxical  failure  in  1956  of  reproduction  to  return  to 
the  1954,  or  higher,  levels,  together  with  the  apparent  stunting,  also  fits 
the  conclusions  discussed  earher.  The  effects  of  increased  density  on  lacta- 
tion and  subsequent  growth  of  the  young  and  in  turn  their  inability  to 
lactate  adequately  were  discussed  earlier,  and  attention  was  called  to  the 
profound  long-range  effects  of  increased  density  on  the  animals  experiencing 
the  increase  as  well  as  two  generations  of  their  progeny  (Chitty,  1955; 
Christian  and  LeMunyan,  1958) .  The  red-backed  voles  which  overwintered 
until  the  spring  of  1956  had  been  subjected  to  increased  density  and  had 
experienced  its  effects.  It  was  noted  that  these  voles  were  unable  to  mature 
in  the  year  of  their  birth  and  were  stunted.  The  following  year  their  repro- 
ductive performance  and  that  of  their  offspring  reflected  a  similar  situation, 
closely  resembling  the  effects  of  increased  density  on  laboratory  popula- 
tions. The  population  dechne  was  consequently  due  to  the  high  mortahty 
of  those  young  born  during  the  period  of  maximum  density  and  probably 
reflects  decreased  resistance  to  a  variety  of  stimuli  coincident  with  increased 
activation  of  adaptive  mechanisms.  A  very  high  rate  of  infant  and  juvenile 
mortality  was  a  major  factor  in  the  cessation  of  growth  in  laboratory  popu- 
lations, but  in  these  there  was  no  appreciable  mortaUty  of  the  adults. 


314  J-  J-  Christian 

There  is  a  continuous  high  rate  of  mortality  of  all  age  groups  in  natural 
populations  of  these  small  mammals  so  that  a  failure  of  reproduction  and 
a  sharply  increased  mortality  rate  of  juvenile  animals  make  a  collapse  of 
the  population  easily  understood.  The  prolonged  recovery  from  this  decline 
also  can  be  attributed  to  the  effects  of  increased  density,  apparently  through 
defects  in  lactation  as  well  as  other  endocrine  reproductive  functions,  al- 
though the  details  of  these  mechanisms  require  much  more  investigation. 
It  should  be  noted  in  these  populations;  that  while  all  phases  of  reproduc- 
tion were  affected  the  particular  functional  aspect  which  was  most  severely 
affected  appeared  to  depend  on  the  level  of  density  and  on  the  age  of  the 
animals  involved — the  young  being  most  severely  affected,  as  was  the  case 
in  laboratory  populations.  It  appears  that  the  responses  of  this  natural 
population  to  density  were  identical  in  every  respect  to  those  seen  in  labora- 
tory populations  with  the  exception  of  the  ever-present  mortality  which 
occurs  in  the  wild. 

Reproductive  function  e\ddently  is  inversely  related  to  density  in  natural 
populations  of  red-backed  voles  {Clethrionomys) ,  and  the  inhibition  of  re- 
production and  growth  probably  resulted  from  a  suppression  of  the  secre- 
tion by  the  anterior  pituitary  of  gonadotropins  and  growth  hormone.  There 
is  no  other  explanation  evident  which  fits  the  known  facts,  even  though 
there  was  no  direct  assessment  of  pituitary  function.  The  apparent  paradox 
of  a  low  population  exhibiting  a  marked  inhibition  of  reproduction  and 
growth  following  a  decline  appears  to  result  from  the  prolonged  effects  of 
high  population  densities  on  these  animals.  These  conclusions  derived  from 
Kalela's  studies  carry  particular  weight,  as  food,  and  apparently  other 
environmental  factors,  appear  not  to  have  been  causative  agents.  Kalela 
(1957)  discusses  the  social  problems  in  these  populations. 

A  quite  similar  study  was  conducted  on  Microtus  montanus  (Hoffmann, 
1958)  with  comparable  results.  This  population  was  followed  for  3  years 
and  there  was  a  peak  between  the  second  and  third  breeding  seasons  which 
was  followed  by  a  marked  decline  in  the  size  of  the  population.  The  popula- 
tion density  was  moderate  in  the  spring  of  1952,  increased  during  the  sum- 
mer and  early  autumn,  and  was  followed  by  the  annual  decline  in  density 
subsequent  to  the  annual  cessation  of  breeding.  The  population  in  the 
spring  of  1953  began  moderately  and  increased  sharply  until  September. 
The  spring  population  of  1954  was  higher  than  in  1953,  but  the  population 
declined  rather  than  increased  during  the  breeding  season  and  was  at  a 
very  low  level  in  the  spring  of  1955.  Thus  the  history  of  this  population  of 
Microtus  montanus  was  in  many  respects  similar  to  that  of  Kalela's  popula- 
tion of  Clethrionomys.  The  published  data  indicate  that  Microtus  montanus 
may  differ  from  most  other  small  mammals  in  that  litter  size  remains 
constant  with  respect  to  age,  weight,  and  parity  (Hoffmann,  1958) .  There- 


2.  Endocrines  and  Populations  315 

fore  changes  in  mean  litter  size  apparently  do  not  need  to  be  corrected  for 
these  variables.  During  each  breeding  season  of  the  study  there  was  an 
appreciable  decline  in  litter  size  as  measured  by  embryos  and  uterine  im- 
plantation scars  and  this  decrease  was  also  paralleled  by  a  decline  in  the 
number  of  corpora  lutea.  There  was  also  an  increase  in  the  general  level  of 
the  population  for  comparable  months  from  1952  to  1953.  This  general  in- 
crease coincided  with  an  overall  decline  in  mean  litter  size  which,  however, 
was  not  significant.  These  changes  were  inversely  related  to  the  annual 
increase  in  population  density  for  the  first  two  years.  However,  the  popula- 
tion declined  sharply  from  spring  to  fall  in  the  third  year  and  yet  the  mean 
litter  size  also  declined. 

Prenatal  mortality  was  directly  related  to  population  density  in  the  first 
breeding  season,  inversely  in  the  third  and  bore  no  conspicuous  relationship 
in  the  second.  Prenatal  mortality  was  greatest  after  the  population  had 
declined  in  September  1954.  The  mean  prevalence  of  pregnancy  also  de- 
clined with  each  successive  year. 

The  peak  of  the  population  was  evidently  in  September  of  1953,  and  the 
decline  followed  shortly  thereafter.  Hoffmami  noted  that  at  this  time  the 
proportion  of  fertile  males  began  to  decline  and  at  the  same  time  24%  of 
the  mature  females  weighed  less  than  33  gm.  This  figure  increased  to  ap- 
proximately 45%  in  June  and  July  of  the  following  season.  The  proportion 
of  mature  males  weighing  less  than  35  gm.  also  rose  to  reach  a  maximum  of 
33%  in  July  of  1954.  These  morphologic  functional  changes  resemble  those 
observed  by  Kalela  and  probably  reflect  suppression  of  growth  at  high 
densities.  The  fact  that  there  was  a  decline  in  male  fertility  as  well  as  in  the 
incidence  of  pregnancy  further  supports  such  a  conclusion.  Hoffman  called 
attention  to  the  fact  that  prenatal  mortality  was  lowest  at  peak  densities 
and  highest  the  following  year.  However,  examination  of  his  data 
reveals  that  the  proportion  of  multiparous  females  rose  steadily  throughout 
the  1953  season  to  reach  100%  in  September,  undoubtedly  accounting  for 
for  the  low  proportion  of  prenatal  losses  for  two  reasons:  (1)  it  is  likely 
that  the  older  parous  females  suffer  less  prenatal  loss  at  any  time;  and  (2) 
more  important,  they  probably  represent  the  dominant  animals  and  there- 
fore those  least  affected  by  high  densities  in  accordance  with  the  results  of 
laboratory  experiments  (Retzlaff,  1938;  Christian  and  LeMunyan,  1958). 
The  age  of  the  animals  could  not  be  determined  in  these  studies ;  immature 
animals  were  classed  as  such  and  were  not  further  divided  into  those  old 
enough  to  have  matured  and  young  animals.  Had  it  been  possible  to  deter- 
mine age,  it  is  likely  that  these  results  would  have  been  more  decisive. 
There  was  a  low  mortality  of  weanlings  and  juveniles  during  the  period  of 
build-up  in  this  population,  but  apparently  there  was  a  marked  increase  in 
mortality  in  all  age  groups  during  the  decline. 


316  /•  /•  Christian 

These  data,  like  those  from  the  preceding  study,  indicate  that  the  effects 
of  high  densities  on  reproduction  and  growth  persist  for  some  time  after 
the  peak  has  been  reached  and  passed.  It  should  be  remembered  that  all 
animals  beginning  the  breeding  season  after  a  peak  are  sur^-ivors  from  the 
peak  densities.  Although  the  highest  numerical  size  of  Hoffmann's  popula- 
tion was  in  September  of  1953,  the  actual  peak  could  just  as  easily  have 
been  the  relatively  high  spring  population  of  1954.  The  data  suggest  this 
to  be  the  case,  as  there  was  a  brief  period  at  the  beginning  of  the  1954 
(June)  breeding  season  in  which  prenatal  and  infant  mortality  were  lower 
than  usual  and  then  climbed  sharply.  Hoffmann  concluded  that  ovulation 
rate  and  Utter  size  tend  to  vary  inversely  with  population  density  and  thus 
to  run  counter  to  the  cychc  trend  of  the  population,  except  during  the 
crash  decUne  period  in  Microtus  montanus,  when  lowered  Utter  size  may 
contribute  to  the  drop  in  density.  He  also  concluded  that  speed  of  attain- 
ment of  reproductive  maturity  remained  rather  constant  despite  changes 
in  density,  which  is  quite  the  reverse  of  Kalela's  results  and  conclusions. 
This  conclusion  is,  however,  based  on  the  assumption  that  there  is  no 
inhibition  of  growth  or  that  it  is  totally  independent  of  changes  in  reproduc- 
tive function.  Since  the  age  of  these  voles  could  not  be  determined,  age 
was  assumed  to  be  reflected  in  body  weight. 

In  view  of  the  likelihood  that  inhibition  of  growth  and  reproduction  are 
not  independent,  but  rather  are  two  manifestations  of  one  reaction,  as  we 
have  seen  for  Clethrionomys  as  weU  as  for  Mus  in  experimental  populations, 
the  conclusion  is  not  justified  that  the  speed  of  attainment  of  maturity 
remains  constant  in  the  presence  of  changes  in  density.  Furthermore, 
Hoffman's  figures  indicate  that  a  high  proportion  of  mature  animals  were 
much  smaller  in  peak  and  declining  populations  than  usually  found  at 
lower  densities.  Of  course  these  data  could  be  interpreted  to  indicate  an 
early  attainment  of  maturity  with  respect  to  size,  but  this  interpretation  is 
most  unUkely  in  view  of  the  other  data  on  reproduction  and  the  results  of 
other  experiments.  This  problem  could  be  settled  by  determining  the  age 
of  these  animals;  a  problem  ah-eady  discussed  at  some  length. 

Chitty  (1952)  also  studied  a  population  of  voles  {Microtus  agrestis)  and 
obtained  results  similar  to  those  of  Kalela.  During  the  peak  breeding  season 
of  1937  he  found  that  young  males  did  not  increase  in  weight  above  22.2 
gm.,  whereas  normally  adult  males  seldom  weigh  less  than  22.3  gm.  after 
May.  Young  females  weighed  more  than  22.2  gm.  only  while  pregnant; 
otherwise  the  heavier  females  in  this  population  were  old  adults  with  a  few 
exceptions  in  September  and  October.  The  prevalence  of  pregnancy  in  the 
peak  year  was  apparently  less  than  in  other  years  and  appeared  to  be  due 
mainly  to  a  failure  of  maturation  in  the  young  of  the  year,  as  older  animals 
exhibited  a  uniformly  high  pregnancy  rate.  In  one  area  all  age  groups  ceased 


2.  Endocrines  and  Populations  317 

breeding  in  August  of  the  peak  summer  and  the  survivors  did  not  breed 
the  following  spring  after  the  population  had  declined.  Other  populations 
which  were  at  peak  density  the  following  year  continued  to  breed  from 
May  to  October  and  survivors  bred  again  the  following  year,  after  the 
dechne,  although  no  recovery  in  population  occurred.  D.  Chitty  concluded 
that  lowered  fertility  was  not  invariably  associated  with  a  decline  in  num- 
bers. The  decreases  in  size  in  all  these  populations  primarily  was  due  to  an 
excessive  juvenile  mortality  before  August  which  was  attributed  to  some 
adverse  effect  of  high  population  density  on  the  ability  of  the  young  to 
survive. 

Hoffmann  (1958)  also  studied  a  population  of  Microtus  calif ornicus  and 
noted  a  general  inverse  relationship  between  density  and  reproductive 
function  which  occurs  on  a  seasonal  basis.  In  other  words,  the  annual  in- 
crease in  the  population  also  has  density  effects  on  its  members.  Fitch  (1957) 
noted  a  decline  in  litter  size  in  Microtus  ochrog aster  with  progression 
of  the  breeding  season  and  increase  in  the  population,  as  noted  by  other 
authors,  but  he  attributed  this  to  an  increasing  number  of  young  of  the 
year  reaching  maturity  and  having  small  first  litters.  However,  this  explana- 
tion does  not  account  completely  for  the  observed  progressive  decline  in  the 
population.  A  similar  relationship  has  also  been  noted  in  muskrats  (Ondatra 
zibethica)  (Errington,  1948,  1951,  1954a).  Errington  (1954a,  1957)  has  also 
shown  that  reproduction  may  be  depressed  during  the  low  years  following 
high  densities  and  increase  during  an  increasing  phase  of  the  population. 
The  depressed  reproduction  probably  is  another  example  of  the  prolonged 
effects  of  high  population  densities  on  succeeding  generations. 

The  pregnancy  rate  was  high  early  in  breeding  season  during  the  peak 
year  in  a  population  of  voles  (Microtus  montanus)  studied  by  Adams,  Bell, 
and  Moore  (Christian,  1959b) ,  but  dropped  precipitously  after  June  so  that 
the  percentage  of  pregnancy  was  below  30%  for  August  and  September — 
months  of  peak  breeding  activity  in  subsequent  years  with  lower  popula- 
tion densities. 

Hamilton  (1937)  reported  an  increase  in  litter  size  with  increasing  popu- 
lation density  and  a  decrease  in  litter  size  during  the  decline.  He  also  re- 
ported increased  intra-uterine  mortality.  However,  although  the  ages  of 
these  animals  were  not  determined,  it  is  probable  that  the  age  composition 
of  the  population  shifted  so  that  it  is  consisted  of  a  higher  proportion  of 
older  animals  as  the  peak  approached.  The  litter  size  of  Microtus  pennsyl- 
vanicus  increases  with  parity  (Hatfield,  1935;  Martin,  1956;  Fitch,  1957) 
differing  from  M.  montanus  in  this  respect;  so  that  the  increase  in  mean 
Utter  size  could  reflect  such  an  occurrence.  The  decline  in  litter  size  during 
the  decrease  in  population  size  is  consistent  with  the  preceding  studies. 

Odum  (1955)  also  reports  a  greater  mean  Htter  size  for  Sigmodon  in 


318  /.  /.  Christian 

"high"  density  years  than  in  "low"  density  years.  Once  again  weight  was 
used  as  an  age  criterion  and  so  the  possibihty  exists  that  Odum's  figures  for 
age  composition  in  "low"  versus  "high"  density  populations  may  reflect 
complete  suppression  of  reproduction  and  growth  in  young  animals  at  high 
densities,  rather  than  a  shift  in  age  composition  toward  younger  animals  as 
his  figures  suggest. 

Tanaka  (1956)  reported  complete  inhibition  of  reproductive  function  in 
Anteliomys  smithii  {Clethriononiys  smithii)  irrespective  of  the  age  or  sex 
of  the  voles  during  a  season  of  peak  densities.  The  reproductive  organs  were 
in  a  condition  of  complete  quiescence.  The  possibility  that  a  deficiency  of 
food  may  have  been  instrumental  in  producing  these  effects  cannot  be 
ruled  out.  However,  Tanaka  (1956,  1957)  points  out  that  maximum  den- 
sities, or  vole  "outbreaks,"  may  occur  either  in  concurrence  with  or  inde- 
pendently of  widespread  flowering  of  bamboo  grasses.  The  fact  that  the 
voles  apparently  were  equally  numerous  and  showed  similar  reproductive 
inhibition  regardless  of  whether  they  were  living  in  areas  of  living  or  areas 
of  dead  bamboo  strongly  suggests  that  the  inhibition  of  reproduction  was 
density  dependent. 

Lemmings,  because  of  the  conspicuousness  of  their  population  cycles, 
have  been  the  subjects  of  study  by  a  number  of  investigators.  Lenimus 
trimucronatus  has  been  reported  to  experience  a  complete  suppression  of 
reproduction  during  periods  of  peak  densities  of  their  populations  (Rausch, 
1950) .  In  the  spring  of  1949,  prior  to  a  precipitous  decline  in  their  popula- 
tion, there  was  an  absence  of  young  animals  and  no  evidence  of  reproduc- 
tive activity:  none  were  pregnant,  there  were  no  uterine  implantation  scars, 
few  females  had  perforate  vaginas,  and  the  testes  of  the  males  were  small 
and  incompletely  descended.  Similar  results  were  obtained  by  Barkalow 
(1952)  in  a  study  of  the  tundra  mouse  (Microtus  oeconom.us)  and  lemmings 
(Lemmus  trimucronatus  and  Dicrostonyx  groenlandicus)  at  Barter  Island, 
Alaska.  Barkalow  in  summarizing  the  results  of  his  and  other  studies  on 
vole  and  lemming  populations  in  the  Arctic  states  that  cessation  of  breeding 
prior  to  and  during  a  cyclic  decline  is  in  his  opinion  a  population  character- 
istic and  that  large  litters  are  characteristic  of  the  build-up  phase  of  a 
population  cycle,  especially  in  its  early  stages.  Rausch  (1950)  stated  that 
although  there  was  a  reduction  in  the  tundra  vegetation  there  was  no  sug- 
gestion that  the  decimation  of  the  population  resulted  from  a  deficiency  of 
food.  Thompson  (1955)  takes  the  view  that  exhaustion  of  the  supply  of 
food  and  cover  is  the  important  factor,  and  well  it  may  be  in  a  local  situa- 
tion. However,  the  bulk  of  the  evidence  suggests  that  a  limited  supply  of 
food  is  not  usually  the  factor  limiting  population  growth.  Lemmings  have 
an  annual  decline  in  reproduction  with  the  annual  increase  of  density,  espe- 
cially in  prepeak  or  peak  years  of  density  similar  to  that  seen  in  the  other 


2.  Eudocrines  and  Populations  319 

microtines  discussed  (Pitelka,  1957b).  Pitelka  (19o7a,  b)  also  makes  the 
point  that  the  cessation  of  reproduction  at  peak  densities  may  occur  and 
produce  a  decline  in  the  lemmings  before  their  predators  have  a  chance  to 
exploit  the  lemming  population.  Lemming  populations  have  not  been 
studied  for  a  long  enough  period  of  time  or  with  sufficiently  detailed  evalua- 
tion of  the  various  factors  which  might  inhibit  reproduction  to  warrant  any 
conclusions  at  present.  However,  it  is  apparent  that  there  can  be  inhibition 
of  reproduction  with  increased  density,  but  the  extent  to  which  food  sup- 
plies may  also  contribute  has  not  been  sufficiently  appraised.  The  evidence 
presently  available  suggests  that  failure  of  reproduction  in  response  to  ex- 
treme densities  is  the  major  cause  of  the  cyclic  declines  in  lemming  popula- 
tions. 

Many  studies  indicating  a  decline  in  reproduction  with  high  population 
densities  in  other  species,  such  as  hares  (Preble,  1908;  MacLulich,  1937), 
but  in  none  of  these  is  it  possible  to  ascribe  the  alteration  in  reproduction 
solely  to  density  factors,  even  though  it  may  well  be  that  such  is  the  case. 
Many  other  studies  could  be  listed  which  illustrate  the  same  sort  of  thing, 
but  no  useful  purpose  would  be  served  by  doing  so  here.  More  recent  studies 
with  Microtus  pennsylvamcus  indicate  that  in  this  species  the  annual  re- 
productive cycle  is  governed  by  population  density,  but  this  does  not  appear 
to  be  true  for  Peromyscus  manicidatus  (Christian,  1961). 

The  main  point  of  interest  is  that  reproduction  does  respond  to  changes 
in  population  density  in  natural  populations  of  voles  and  rats  and  probably 
in  other  .species  as  well.  In  some  studies  there  was  a  reciprocal  relationship 
between  adrenocortical  and  reproductive  function,  in  others  only  one  or 
the  other  of  these  general  responses  was  studied,  but  the  results  conform 
to  this  general  relationship.  It  seems  undeniable,  especially  in  the  light  of 
studies  on  laboratory  populations,  that  physiologic  adaptive  responses  are 
operative  in  natural  populations  and  are  reflected  by  curtailment  of  repro- 
duction and  growth  and  stimulation  of  increased  pituitary-adrenocortical 
activity  in  natural  populations.  However,  detailed  and  well  correlated 
studies  on  a  variety  of  species  still  are  required  and  for  a  variety  of  different 
populations  in  order  to  assess  the  relative  importance  of  these  mechanisms 
in  curtailing  free-for-all  population  growth. 

3.  Disease  Resistance  and  Mortality 

One  of  the  major  points  of  the  hypothesis  that  physiologic  mechanisms 
could  control  population  growth  as  a  feedback  system  acting  in  response  to 
changes  in  density  was  that  the  sudden  mass  mortality  seen  at  the  end  of  a 
build-up  of  populations  to  peak  densities  could  be  accounted  by  exhaustion 
of  the  adaptive  responses  (Christian,  1950b).  It  was  suggested  that  the 


320  /.  /.  Christian 

symptoms  so  frequently  observed  (Hamilton,  1937;  Green  and  Larson, 
1938 ;  Green,  et  al.,  1939)  were  the  result  of  hypoglycemic  shock  as  a  result  of 
pituitary-adrenocortical  exhaustion.  The  subject  remains  at  that  point  to- 
day, except  that  Frank  (1953)  has  succeeded  in  showing  that  hypoglycemic 
shock,  which  is  uncorrected  by  epinephrine  but  is  corrected  by  glucose  in- 
jection, occurs  in  Microtus.  Frank  (1953)  also  succeeded  in  producing  mass 
mortality  from  hypoglycemic  shock  by  crowding  voles  in  enclosures.  How- 
ever, he  believes  that  in  addition  to  increased  density  alone,  which  sets  the 
conditions  for  a  mass  mortality,  a  trigger,  such  as  competition  for  food  must 
also  be  present.  This  seems  a  reasonable  suggestion  which  perhaps  can  be 
modijfied  to  be  included  in  density-dependent  factors.  On  the  basis  of 
Frank's  and  other  experimental  data  it  appears  that  competition  for  any 
factor  is  also  a  matter  of  relative  population  density  and  inseparable  from 
it.  In  this  case  it  seems  that  the  voles  were  reducing  the  environmental 
capacity  at  high  densities  and  were  therefore  themselves  setting  up  the 
conditions  for  increasing  competition  among  themselves  above  the  level 
that  was  already  present. 

It  has  been  mentioned  that  mass  mortality  in  sika  deer  was  probably  a 
result  of  electrolyte  imbalances  brought  about  by  prolonged  adrenocortical 
hyperactivity.  There  were  indications  of  a  prominent  role  of  the  zona 
glomerulosa,  presumably  with  increased  secretion  of  aldosterone,  to  explain 
the  evidence  of  potassium  deficiency  (Christian  et  al.,  1960;  Christian, 
1963). 

Frank  (1953)  was  able  to  eliminate  infectious  disease  and  climatic 
factors  as  causative  agents  in  precipitating  the  mortality  in  these  animals, 
as  were  Adams,  Bell,  and  Moore  (Christian,  1959b)  in  Microtus  montanus. 
Rausch  (1950)  also  indicated  that  neither  parasitism  nor  infectious  disease 
could  account  for  the  sudden  decline  in  the  lemming  population  he  was 
studying.  In  a  study  of  mortality  in  an  experimentally  induced  epidemic 
in  Norway  rats,  Davis  and  Jensen  (1952)  found  that  mortality  rate  was  not 
significantly  changed  and  probably  would  be  so  only  under  special  circimi- 
stances.  A  precipitous  population  decline  due  to  mass  mortality  from  hypo- 
glycemic shock  is  probably  only  one  of  several  mechanisms  which  may 
operate  to  reduce  population  density  drastically  and  it  may  be  a  rather 
special  occurrence.  Our  knowledge  of  the  physiologic  mechanisms  involved 
in  invoking  "shock  disease"  in  natural  or  experimental  populations  is  little 
better  understood  now  than  previously.  It  was  originally  postulated  that 
adrenocortical  exhaustion  was  the  cause,  but  it  is  just  as  likely,  if  not  more 
so,  that  readily  mobilizable  sources  of  glycogen  and  glucose  become  totally 
exhausted  with  continued  overstimulation  of  the  adrenal  medulla  and  cor- 
tex. There  may  be  exhaustion  of  other  parts  of  the  responsive  system,  such 
as  the  pituitary,  hypothalamus,  or  of  the  adrenal  medulla.  None  of  these 


2.  Endocrines  and  Populations  321 

possibilities  has  been  explored  and  in  retrospect  the  idea  of  attributing 
hypoglycemic  shock  solely  to  adrenocortical  exhaustion  seems  naive.  That 
the  hypoglycemic  shock  syndrome  does  occur  cannot  be  denied,  but  its 
physiological  causes  remain  to  be  determined.  Such  indirect  evidence  as  is 
available  indicates  that  it  is  a  very  complex  affair.  Mice  and  voles  have 
been  observed  to  die  of  hypoglycemic  shock  shortly  after  being  placed 
placed  together  in  groups  (Frank,  1953;  Christian,  1955a),  but  if  moribund 
animals  are  removed  from  their  cagemates,  they  recover  (Frank,  1953;  J.  J. 
Christian,  unpublished) .  The  rapidity  with  which  this  occurs  suggests  some- 
thing other  than  cortical  exhaustion,  perhaps  medullary  exhaustion  or 
exhaustion  of  readily  available  glycogen  reserves.  In  addition,  there  is 
evidence  that  adrenal  cortical  exhaustion  may  not  occur  per  se,  but  may 
be  an  apparent  result  of  exhaustion  elsewhere  in  the  adaptive  system  (Ro- 
senfeld,  1958). 

At  the  present  time  it  appears  that  a  failiu-e  in  reproductive  function 
may  account  for  a  decline  in  a  population  much  more  frequently  than  shock. 
It  has  been  observed  in  several  species  that  the  young  born  during  peaks 
of  density  appear  to  be  much  more  susceptible  to  various  mortality  factors, 
and  their  deaths  largely  account  for  precipitous  population  declines  (Green 
and  Evans,  1940a,  b,  c;  Chitty,  1952,  1954;  Godfrey,  1955).  This  dispro- 
portionate mortality  of  young  animals  may  result  from  three  different 
factors  that  have  already  been  discussed:  (1)  they  are  the  subordinate 
animals  in  a  population  and  therefore,  as  we  have  seen,  are  much  more 
severely  exposed  to  sociopsychologic  pressures  than  older  and  more  domi- 
nant animals;  (2)  they  appear  to  be  more  sensitive  to  these  pressures  and 
respond  accordingly;  and  (3)  they  are  adversely  affected  by  deficient 
lactation  on  the  part  of  their  mothers,  and  then-  progeny  in  turn  are 
affected.  The  combination  of  these  factors  would  be  expected  to  have  pro- 
found effects  on  the  young,  and  we  have  already  seen  that  their  reproduc- 
tive function  is  severely  depressed.  Excessive  stimulation  of  the  adaptive 
responses  in  these  animals  would  also  be  expected  to  result  in  exhaustion 
and  shock.  Frank  (1953)  has  shown  that  socially  inferior  voles  are  the 
ones  primarily  affected  and  that  succumb  to  shock. 

In  addition  to  these  effects  of  social  pressure  and  increased  activity  of 
physiologic  adaptive  responses,  one  would  anticipate  a  marked  decline  in 
resistance  to  infection,  parasitism,  or  other  harsh  environment  experiences. 
It  is  not  surprising  that  epidemics  are  frequently  observed  following  peak 
densities  and  a  general  decrease  in  host  resistance.  It  is  particularly  signifi- 
cant that  a  variety  of  agents  may  be  found  to  produce  death  in  a  given 
population,  apparently  as  a  result  of  decreased  resistance  (Chitty,  1954) . 

It  is  not  the  intent  of  this  account  to  dwell  on  the  causes  of  mortality  in 
natural  populations  except  insofar  as  a  decreased  resistance  may  result  from 


322  J-  J'  Christian 

increased  physiologic  adaptation  to  increased  density.  A  causal  relation- 
ship between  population  density  and  parasitism  in  hares  is  suggested  by 
the  studies  of  Erickson  (1944).  Parasites  were  scarce  when  population 
densities  were  low  and  maximiun  parasitism  was  found  in  populations  of 
hares  at  peak  densities.  The  hare  populations  declined  but  the  parasites 
remained  abundant  in  the  surviving  animals.  There  was  no  evident  correlation 
between  shock  disease  and  the  parasites  in  these  hares  other  than  that 
parasitism  was  at  high  levels  when  the  population  began  to  decline  owing 
to  shock.  Similarly,  a  close  correspondence  between  the  level  of  parasitism 
of  rabbits  by  Eimeria  stiedae  and  population  density  was  found  in  New 
Zealand  (Whittle,  1955)  and  it  was  suggested  that  the  differences  in  the 
incidence  of  severe  infections  were  correlated  with  differences  in  host  re- 
sistence,  which  in  turn  may  have  been  a  function  of  host  density  (Bull, 
1955,  1957,  1958).  It  is  of  interest  that  young  rabbits  were  the  ones  pri- 
marily affected  in  these  populations  (Tj^ndale-Biscoe  and  Williams,  1955; 
Bull,  1958).  In  a  study  on  parasitism  by  lice  on  populations  of  Microtus 
penrisylvanicvs  and  Peromyscus  maniculatus  it  was  found  that  the  level  of 
parasitism  varied  with  season  in  both  the  voles  and  the  deer  mice,  but  that 
the  size  of  the  louse  population  changed  significantly  with  season  only  in 
the  male  voles  (Cook  and  Beer,  1958).  It  was  suggested  that  the  spring 
maximum  in  the  louse  populations  on  the  male  voles  came  at  the  period  of 
greatest  "stress"  and  therefore  the  increase  in  the  louse  population  at  this 
time  resulted  from  decreased  host  resistance. 

The  evidence  for  a  decrease  in  host  resistance  to  specific  diseases  or 
parasitism  in  natural  populations  is  not  conclusive,  but  is  certainly  sugges- 
tive. However,  there  can  be  little  (luestion  that  there  are  severe  physiologi- 
cal derangements  associated  with  high  population  densities  which  result  in 
high  mortality  rates  in  voles,  lemmings,  and  hares  (Green  and  Evans, 
1940b;  Elton,  1942;  Rausch,  1950;  Chitty,  1952,  1954;  Frank,  1953;  God- 
frey, 1955),  and  that  this  mortality  cannot  be  accounted  for  by  infectious 
disease,  even  though  the  prevalence  of  infection  and  parasitism  may  be 
increased.  It  is  equally  evident  that  young  animals  are  more  severely  af- 
fected than  older  animals  and  that  the  effects  are  prolonged  and  account 
for  continued  declines  in  populations  for  appreciable  periods  following  a 
sharp  reduction  in  population  density  (Chitty,  1952,  1954,  1955;  Christian 
and  LeMunyan,  1958;  Christian,  1959a,  b). 

In  summary,  there  is  much  indirect  evidence  indicating  that  a  decline  of 
a  population  from  peak  densities  may  be  caused  by  a  density-dependent 
activation  of  physiologic  adaptive  mechanisms  and  exerting  their  effects 
in  one  or  all  of  several  ways: 

1.  There  may  be  direct  mass  mortality  due  to  physiologic  exhaustion 
following  prolonged,  excessive  stimulation  of  the  adaptive  mechanisms. 


2.  Endocrines  and  Populations  323 

2.  There  may  be  increased  mortality  due  to  parasitism  or  infectious 
disease  due  to  decreased  host  resistance  (presumably  from  suppression  of 
inflammatory  and  immune  defense  mechanisms  by  increased  adreoncortical 
activity) . 

3.  There  may  be  partial  or  complete  inhibition  of  reproductive  function, 
especially  in  the  younger  animals  and  with  prolonged  effects  on  the  next 
generations  of  progeny,  apparently  initiated  by  deficient  lactation  on  the 
part  of  mothers  subjected  to  mcreased  density,  but  also  owing  to  the  fact 
that  the  young  probably  are  most  severely  affected  by  increased  density. 

It  is  assumed  that  in  the  absence  of  mass  mortality  normal  or  moderately 
increased  mortality  rates  can  produce  striking  declines  in  density  when 
reproduction  is  partially  or  wholly  suppressed.  The  bulk  of  evidence  in  the 
literature  indicates  that  this  is  more  usually  the  case  than  mass  mortality. 
Furthermore,  food  shortages,  epidemics,  or  predation  may  be  the  immediate 
cause  of  the  mortality,  although  it  is  equally  clear  that  the  mortality  still 
occurs  when  these  factors  are  absent  (Elton,  1942;  Rausch,  1950;  Chitty, 
1954;  Godfrey,  1955;  Pitelka,  1957a,  b).  It  is  also  possible  that  some  pre- 
cipitating factors  may  act  through  the  physiologic  adaptive  mechanisms  by 
increasing  intraspecific  strife,  as  was  shown  to  be  the  case  in  experimental 
populations  of  voles  (Frank,  1953). 

4.  Summary:  Natural  Populations 

There  is  considerable  evidence  to  indicate  that  there  is  a  density-de- 
pendent increase  in  the  activity  of  physiologic  adaptive  mechanisms  in 
natural  populations  of  Norway  rats,  voles  {Microtus,  Clethrionomys,  Dicro- 
stonijx,  Lemmus,  Ondatra),  hares  (Leptis),  rabbits  {Onjctolagus) ,  white- 
footed  mice  (Peromijscus) ,  and  probably  other  species.  A  relationship  be- 
tween population  density  and  adrenocortical  activity  has  been  demon- 
strated for  Norway  rats,  meadow  voles  {Microtus  niontanus  and  M. 
pennsylvanicus) ,  white-footed  mice  (Peromyscus  leucopus),  and  sika  deer 
(Cervus  nippon).  An  inverse  relationship  between  density  and  reproductive 
function  exists  for  Norway  rats,  red-backed  voles  (Clethrionomys) ,  meadow 
voles  {Microtus  agrestis,  M.  montanus,  M.  californicus,  M.  pennsylvanicus), 
muskrats  {Ondatra),  cotton  rats  {Sigmodon),  and  hares  (Lepus).  However, 
in  few  of  these  studied  were  reproductive  function  and  adrenocortical  func- 
tion correlated  in  the  same  population.  Furthermore  adequate  assessment 
of  other  envu-onmental  factors  has  not  been  made.  In  many  investigations 
there  were  no  adequate  means  of  determining  age,  so  that  a  true  indication 
of  the  degree  of  inhibition  of  reproductive  function  was  not  possible,  espe- 
cially with  a  simultaneous  suppression  of  growth.  Determinations  of 
age  should  be  possible  in  a  variety  of  species  if  a  concerted  effort  is  made 
to  find  the  proper  criteria.  There  is  evidence  that,  as  in  the  laboratory, 


324  /.  /.  Christian 

there  is  suppression  of  lactation,  as  well  as  of  other  reproductive  functions, 
with  increased  density  which  may  account  for  the  prolonged  effects  of  high 
density  on  the  young.  However,  the  effects  of  high  density  may  also  be 
exerted  on  the  young  animals  directly.  There  is  also  evidence  that  resistance 
to  disease  and  parasites  is  decreased,  especially  m  the  young,  at  high  popula- 
tion densities.  In  extreme  cases,  there  may  be  mass  mortalities  of  popula- 
tions due  to  hypoglycemic  shock,  presumably  following  exhaustion  of  some 
part  of  the  entire  physiologic  mechanism  or  of  the  reserves  of  readily 
available  necessary  metabolites  or  both. 

None  of  the  data  from  natural  populations  is  conclusive  evidence  that 
density-dependent  responses  of  physiologic  adaptive  mechanisms  are  active 
in  the  regulation  and  control  of  the  growth  of  mammalian  populations. 
However,  when  the  available  evidence  is  viewed  in  the  light  of  the  results 
obtained  from  carefully  controlled  studies  on  laboratory  populations,  there 
is  sufficient  reason  to  conclude  that  these  mechanisms  are  operative  in 
natural  mammalian  populations,  and  more  precisely  in  populations  of 
rodents  and  lagomorphs.  It  is  equally  apparent  that  sociopsychologic  pres- 
sures are  the  stimuli  to  the  various  physiologic  responses  involved.  There- 
fore, there  is  a  wide  range  in  responses  in  the  individuals  of  a  population, 
the  subordinate,  and  therefore  younger,  animals  being  the  most  severely 
involved.  Finally,  it  appears  that  these  sociopsychologic-physiologic  mecha- 
nisms are  basic  controlling  factors  in  populations  of  mammals  and  other 
factors  are  secondarily  important  in  most  cases. 

There  is  a  great  need  for  a  coordinated  study  on  a  small  rodent  for  which 
there  are  criteria  for  determming  age  in  which  adrenocortical  function,  re- 
productive function,  resistance  to  disease,  mortality,  social  factors,  and 
enviroilmental  factors,  can  be  adequately  studied  for  a  complete  cycle  of 
the  population.  Until  the  results  of  such  a  study  are  available,  conclusions 
regarding  density-dependent  physiologic  mechanisms  must  lean  heavily  on 
extrapolations  from  data  gathered  from  experimental  populations  in  the 
laboratory.  There  are  in  addition  to  the  general  problem  many  specific 
problems  that  need  investigation.  Many  of  these  have  been  mentioned  m 
the  course  of  the  discussion,  but  a  few  have  not.  A  major  problem  is  what 
factors  determine  where  the  reproductive  process  will  be  affected  in  a 
particular  population.  For  example,  why  is  it  intra-uterine  mortality  in  one 
case,  total  inhibition  of  reproductive  function  in  another,  and  depressed 
lactation  in  a  third?  The  explanations  are  largely  conjectural  at  present, 
but  that  these  differences  exist  cannot  be  questioned,  yet  all  represent  a 
common  mechanism  and  effect:  inhibition  of  reproduction.  The  answer  to 
another  question  may  provide  the  answers  to  those  already  asked :  how  do 
social  pressures  affect  the  individual  members  of  a  population? 


2.  Endocrines  and  Populations  325 

III.  Conclusion 

The  first  part  of  this  chapter  reviewed  briefly  the  endocrine  adaptive 
mechanisms  which  are  of  unquestionable  importance  in  enabling  mammals 
to  meet  and  adapt  to  their  constantly  changing  environment  and  the  visic- 
situdes  of  daily  existence.  The  mechanisms  discussed  are  by  no  means  the 
only  adaptive  responses  which  are  evoked  by  environmental  exigencies, 
but  they  are  the  better  known  ones  and  at  the  present  time  those  which  are 
most  Hkely  to  be  of  interest  to  mammalogists,  ecologists,  and  students  of 
mammalian  population  dynamics.  The  first  part  of  the  chapter  was  in  itself 
a  summary  review  of  these  responses;  so  no  useful  purpose  would  be  served 
by  further  condensing  and  .summarizing  at  this  point.  However,  it  should 
be  emphasized  that  one  is  not  justified  in  regarding  any  single  response,  for 
example  increased  adrenocortical  activity,  as  an  isolated  phenomenon 
complete  unto  itself  and  independent  of  any  other  changes.  Any  adaptive 
response  initiates  and  is  a  part  of  an  extremely  complex  series  of  physio- 
logic changes  which  probably  involve  every  aspect  of  the  host's  physiology 
and  metabolism.  Furthermore,  the  adaptive  responses  are  not  static  affairs, 
but  a  system  of  dynamic  changes  in  the  nature  of  feedback  mechanisms 
which  operate  to  maintain  a  constant  internal  environment  and  life.  As 
stated  earlier,  these  mechanisms  are  undoubtedly  constantly  active  and 
responding  to  even  minor  changes  encountered  in  daily  life.  Emphasis  is 
lent  to  this  statement  by  the  hypoactive  physiologic  state  observed  in 
closely  confined,  inactive,  and  overfed  mammals.  Therefore  one  should 
regard  the  adaptive  responses  that  are  customarily  studied  and  reported 
merely  as  quantitative  deviations  from  "normal"  daily  experience. Finally, 
we  should  regard  physiologic  adaptations  as  flexible  in  that  the  available 
evidence  indicates  that  qualitatively  different  stimuli  probably  do  not 
elicit  qualitatively  similar  responses  even  though  cursory  examination  may 
seem  to  contradict  this  statement.  The  components  of  the  responses  prob- 
ably differ  proportionally  from  each  other  with  differing  stimuli.  It  is  true 
that  certain  organ  systems  are  primarily  responsive  to  the  demands  of 
external  change,  but  their  responses  appear  not  to  be  identical,  either 
quantitatively  or  qualitatively,  to  all  stimuli.  For  example,  we  have  seen 
that  heat  and  cold  elicit  quite  different  responses,  while  emotional  stimuli 
evoke  a  third  and  different  set  of  reactions,  although  all  three  may  have 
certain  features  in  common.  Therefore  a  plea  is  again  made  to  examine 
adaptive  responses  in  the  light  of  the  specific  stimuli  by  which  they  were 
elicited.  Generalization  serves  an  extremely  useful  purpose  conceptually, 
but  it  may  be  misleading  in  the  interpretation  of  results  from  critical 
studies  designed  to  explore  physiologic  adjustments  to  specific  stimuli. 


326  J-    f-   Christiau 

iMiuill}',  what  has  been  presented  here  only  represents  a  selected  stopping 
point  in  a  very  rapidly  advancing  field  of  research  and  must,  perforce,  be 
used  as  such  and  modified  in  the  light  of  newer  developments. 

The  second  part  of  this  chapter  attempted  to  review  critically  the  evi- 
dence from  the  laboratory  and  from  the  field  relating  physiologic  adaptive 
responses  to  changes  in  population  density,  largely  in  response  to  poorly 
defined  sociopsychologic  factors.  There  seems  to  be  little  doubt  that  endo- 
crine adaptive  responses  to  sociopsychologic  pressures  are  of  basic  import- 
ance in  the  regulation  of  population  growth,  at  least  for  a  limited  number 
of  species.  It  also  is  obvious  that  a  great  many  gaps  remain  to  be  filled 
before  the  evidence  for  the  physiologic  regulation  of  natural  populations 
can  be  considered  conclusive  one  way  or  the  other  except  for  a  few  species 
of  rodent.  These  gaps  have  been  pomted  out  in  the  appropriate  portions 
of  the  foregoing  discussions.  Nevertheless,  considerable  support  has  been 
derived  in  the  last  ten  years  for  the  original  hypothesis  that  factors  inti- 
mately related  to  population  density  are  stimuli  to  physiologic  feedback 
mechanisms  and  that  population  growth  and  decline  are  largely  controlled 
by  changes  in  density.  It  has  also  become  evident  that  "density"  in  terms 
of  mammalian  populations  is  related  only  indirectly  to  numbers  of  mammals 
per  unit  of  area,  being  more  directly  related  to  intraspecific  competition, 
social  strife,  sociopsychologic  pressures,  or  whatever  other  comparable 
designation  one  may  choose  to  use  for  the  interactions  between  mammals 
in  the  same  population.  It  is  obvious  that  these  factors  require  a  great  deal 
more  study  in  order  to  define  them  precisely. 

Figure  1  is  presented  as  a  schematic  summary  of  the  physiologic  feedback 
n^gulation  of  population  growth  as  it  is  envisioned  today  in  the  light  of  the 
available  experimental  evidence.  It  should  be  re-emphasized  that  this  is  a 
dynamic  and  flexible  system  and  that  the  importance  of  various  compo- 
nents may  vary  with  respect  to  each  other.  The  broad  ascending  arrows  on 
the  left  of  the  figure  indicate  that  as  population  size  increases,  social  pres- 
sures increase  accordingly.  The  dotted  lines  above  and  below  the  arrow  for 
population  increase  serve  to  indicate  that  the  actual  population  size  may 
vary  considerably  from  population  to  population  for  a  given  degree  of  social 
pressure,  which  we  see  hypothetically  as  the  fundamental  growth-regulating 
and  growth-limiting  factor  in  all  mammalian  populations  and  the  factor  to 
which  physiologic  adaptive  mechanisms  respond.  However,  that  this  factor 
is  always  present  and  always  operative  does  not  necessarily  imply  that  it  is 
always  the  factor  which  limits  population  growth.  Environmental  factors 
probably  operate  through  this  mechanism  by  increasing  or  decreasing  social 
pressures,  although  conclusive  evidence  for  this  statement  presently  is 
lacking.  There  is  good  e^'idence,  howe\^er,  that  the  social  factors  will  limit 
population  growth  despite  an  abundance  of  all  environmental  requirements 
and  that  the  degree  of  social  density  is  related  to  the  behavioral  composition 


2.  Endocrines  and  Populations 


327 


i  lie 

O      *    E  < 

Itul 


o 

z 

5 

w     O" 

O 

;  = 

< 

<=    2  - 

z 

•   c   £ 

2 

< 
o 

z 

^     c     ^ 

"- 

<   -o    - 

328  /•  J-   Christian 

(A  the  population.  As  social  pressure  increases,  it  acts  as  a  progressively 
stronger  stimulus  to  physiologic  adaptation  via  the  mechanisms  on  the 
right  of  the  broken  vertical  line.  These  in  turn  act  to  decrease  reproduction 
and  increase  mortality,  and  therefore  the  rate  of  population  growth  declines 
with  increasing  density  and  eventually  ceases,  as  indicated  by  the"reproduc- 
tion"  and  "mortality"  arrows  on  the  left.  We  have  already  seen  that  repro- 
duction may  cease  altogether  at  maximmii  densities  and  that  mortality 
may  achieve  precipitous  proportions.  An  alteration  in  age  composition  of 
the  population  is  assumed  to  be  inherent  m  these  effects.  The  details  of  the 
various  mechanisms  have  been  discussed  earlier,  and  will  not  be  discussed 
further  here.  However,  it  is  hoped  that  this  diagram  will  help  in  visualizing 
the  dynamic  mterrelationships  between  the  endocrine  responses  in  the 
members  of  the  population  to  changes  in  the  population  as  a  whole.  It  will 
be  observed  that  a  food  deficiency  has  been  shown  as  decreasing  directly 
the  production  of  gonadotropins,  but,  as  we  have  discussed  earlier,  it  may 
serve  to  increase  competition  and  therefore  effective  density  under  some 
circumstances.  Theoretically  it  seems  unlikely  that  a  food  deficiency  would 
reach  proportions  in  natural  populations  which  would  enable  it  to  exert  its 
effects  dhectly  on  the  pituitary  without  competitive  factors  first  becoming 
operative.  This  diagrammatic  hypothesis  appears  to  fit  the  facts  available 
for  a  Imiited  number  of  studies  on  a  few  species  of  rodent,  and  undoubtedly 
it  will  be  necessary  to  modify  it  as  information  from  more  critical  studies 
become  available.  It  is  of  primary  importance  to  obtain  results  from  careful 
studies  on  ungulates,  similar  to  those  already  conducted  on  rodents,  before 
regulation  of  population  growth  of  ungulates  by  sociopsychologic-physio- 
logic  feedback  mechanisms  can  be  considered  to  be  more  than  hypothetical. 
However,  at  present  it  is  very  probable  that  these  mechanisms  are  basically 
important  in  regulating  the  growth  of  populations  of  rodents  and  lago- 
morphs. 

An  attempt  has  been  made  ui  this  chapter  to  point  out  the  areas  where 
critical  data  are  lackuig  and  some  of  the  problems  and  pitfalls  which  may 
be  encountered  in  investigating  these  physiologic  phenomena  in  relation  to 
population  density.  It  is  hoped  that  it  will  serve  as  a  useful  guide  for  further 
research  by  investigators  who  wish  to  explore  the  mechanics  of  population 
dynamics. 

References 

Ackerman,  N.  B.,  and  Arons,  W.  L.  (1958).  The  effect  of  epinephrine  and  norepinephrine 
on  the  acute  thyroid  release  of  thyroid  hormones.  Endocrinology  62,  723-737. 

Agate,  F.  J.,  Jr.  (1952).  The  possible  role  of  the  adrenal  cortex  in  fluid  shifts  in  the  ham- 
ster following  renal  damage.  Ann.  N.  Y.  Acad.  Sci.  55,  404-411. 

Albert,  A.,  Tenney,  A.,  and  Ford,  E.  (1952).  The  effect  of  cortisone  and  corticotropin  on 
the  biologic  decay  of  thyroidal  radioiodine.  Endocrinology  50,  324-326. 


2.  Endocrines  and  Populations  329 

Allen,  J.  C.  (1954).  Effect  of  cortisone  on  the  morphology  of  the  mouse  adrenal  with 

special  reference  to  changes  in  the  X-zone.  Bull.  Johns  Hopkins  Hasp.  94,  349-350. 
Allen,  J.  M.  (1957).  A  chemical  and  histochemical  studj'  of  aliesterase  in  mouse  adrenal 

cortex  under  hormonal  influences  and  altered  sodium  intake.  Endocrinology  61,  368- 

374. 
Alpert,  M.  (1950).  Observations  on  the  histophysiology  of  the  adrenal  gland  of  the 

golden  hamster.  Endocrinologij  46,  166-167. 
Anderson,  C.  H.,  McCally,  M.,  and  Farrell,  G.  L.  (1959).  The  effects  of  atrial  stretch  on 

aldosterone  secretion.  Endocrinology  64,  202-207. 
Anderson,  E.,  and  Haymaker,  W.  (1948a).  Influence  of  the  hypothalamus  on  sexual 

functions,  Part  I.  J.  Am.  Med.  Women's  Assoc.  3,  402-406. 
Anderson,  E.,  and  Haymaker,  W.  (1948b).  Influence  of  the  hypothalamus  on  sexual 

functions,  Part  II.  /.  Am.  Med.  Women's  Assoc.  3,  457-461. 
Andervont,  H.  B.  (1944).  Influence  of  environment  on  mammary  cancer  in  mice.  /. 

Natl.  Cancer  Inst.  10,  579-581. 
Antopol,  W.  (1953).  Anatomical  changes  with  excessive  doses  of  cortisone  in  the  normal 

mouse,  the  pregnant  mouse,  the  fetus,  and  the  offspring.  Ain.  J.  Pathol.  29,  612-613. 
Ashng,  C.  W.,  Reinhardt,  W.  O.,  and  Li,  C.  H.  (1951).  Effects  of  adrenocorticotropic 

hormone  on  body  growth,  visceral  proportions  and  white  blood  cell  counts  of  normal 

and  hypophysectomized  male  rate.  Endocrinology  48,  534-547. 
Aycock,  W.  L.,  and  Ingalls,  T.  H.  (1946).  Maternal  disease  as  a  principle  in  the  epido- 

miology  of  congenital  anomalies.  Am.  J.  Med.  Sci.  212,  366-379. 
Ayres,  P.  J.,  Qould,  R.  P.,  Simpson,  S.  A.  S.,  and  Tait,  J.  F.  (1956).  The  in  vitro  demon- 
stration  of  differential  corticosteroid  production  within   the   ox   adrenal  gland. 

Biochem.  J.  63,  19p. 
Bachman,  R.  (1954).  Die  nebenniere.  In  "Handbuch  der  mikroskopischen  Anatomic 

des  Menschen"  (W.  v.  Mollendorft',  ed.).  Vol.  6.  Springer,  Berlin. 
Badrick,  F.  E.,  Brimblecombe,  R.  W.,  Reiss,  J.  M.,  and  Reiss,  M.  (1954).  The  influence 

of  stress  conditions  on  the  uptake  of  V^^  by  the  rat  thyroid.  /.  Endocrinol.  11, 

305-313. 
Badrick,  F.  E.,  Brimblecombe,  R.  W.,  and  Reiss,  M.  (1955).  Responses  of  hypophy- 

sectomized  rats  to  stress.  J.  Endocrinol.  12,  205-208. 
Bahn,  R.,  Wilson,  H.,  Kedda,  L.,  Furth,  J.,  and  Anderson,  E.  (1957).  Metabohc  changes 

associated  with  transplantable  ACTH-producing  pituitary  tumors  of  the  mouse. 

Can.  J.  Biochem.  and  Physiol.  35,  1057-1065. 
Baker,  B.  L.  (1949).  Modification  of  body  stmcture  by  adrenocortical  secretions  with 

special  reference  to  the  regulation  of  growth.  Am.  Assoc.  Advance.  Sci.  Symposium 

on  Pituitary- Adrenal  Function,  pp.  88-95. 
Baker,  B.  L.  (1952).  A  comparison  of  the  histological  changes  induced  by  experimental 

hyperadrenacorticalism  and  inanition.  Recent  Progr.  in  Hormone  Research  7,  331-373. 
Baker,  B.  L.  (1954).  The  connective  tissue  reaction  around  implanted  pellets  of  steroid 

hormones.  Anat.  Record  119,  529-539. 
Baker,  B.  L.,  and  Abrams,  G.  D.  (1954).  Effect  of  hypophysectomy  on  the  cytology  of 

the  fundii  glands  of  the  stomach  and  on  the  secretion  of  pepsin.  Am.  J.  Physiol. 

177,  409-412. 
Baker,  B.  L.,  and  Bridgman,  R.  M.  (1954).  The  histology  of  the  gastro-intestinal  mucosa 

(rat)  after  adrenalectomy  or  administration  of  adrenocortical  hormones.  Am.  J. 

Anat.  94,  363-397. 
Baker,  B.  L.,  Schairer,  M.  A.,  Ingle,  D.  J.,  and  Li,  C.  H.  (1950).  The. induction  of  in- 
volution in  the  male  reproductive  system  by  treatment  with  adrenocorticotropin. 

Anat.  Record  106,  345-359. 


330  /.  /.   Christian 

Baker,  B.  L.,  Ingle,  1).  J.,  and  Li,  C.  H.  (1951).  The  histology  of  the  lymphoid  organs  of 

rats  treated  with  adrenocorticotropin.  Am.  J.  Anal.  88,  313-349. 
Baker,  J.  R.,  and  Ransom,  R.  M.  (1932).  Factors  affecting  breeding  of  the  field  mouse 

{Microtus  agresiis).  Part  II.  Temperature  and  food.  Pi-oc.  Roy.  Soc.  B112,  39-46. 
Barkalow,  F.  S.,  Jr.  (1952).  Life  history  and  ecologic  observations  on  the  tundra  mouse 

{Microtus  oeconomus)  and  lemmings  (Lemmus  trimucronatus  and  Dicrostonyx  groen- 

landicus)  at  Barter  Island,  Alaska.  J.  Elisha  Mitchell  Sci.  Soc.  68,  199-205. 
Barker,  S.  B.,  and  Schwartz,  H.  S.  (1953).  Further  studies  on  metabolism  of  tissues 

from  thyroxine-injected  rats.  Proc.  Soc.  Exptl.  Biol.  Med.  83,  500-502. 
Barnett,  S.  A.  (1955).  Competition  among  wild  rats.  Nature  175,  126-127. 
Barnett,  S.  A.  (1958).  Physiological  effects  of  "social  stress"  in  wild  rats.  I.  The  adrenal 

corte.x.  J .  Psychosoniat.  Research  3,  1-11. 
Barnett,  S.  A.,  and  Manly,  B.  M.  (1956).  Reproduction  and  growth  of  mice  of  three 

strains,  after  transfer  to  -3°C.  J.  Exptl.  Biol.  33,  325-329. 
Barraclough,  C.  A.  (1961).  Production  of  anovulatory,  sterile  rats  by  single  injections  of 

testosterone  propionate.  Endocrinology  68,  62-67. 
Barraclough,  C.  A.,  and  Gorski,  R.  A.  (1961).  Evidence  that  the  hypothalamus  is  respon- 
sible for  androgen-induced  Sterility  in  the  female  rat.  Endocrinology  68,  68-79. 
Barry,  M.  C,  Eidinoff,  M.  L.,  Dobriner,  K.,  and  Gallagher,  T.  F.  (1952).  The  fate  of  Ci^- 

progesterone  in  mice  and  rats.  Endocrinology  50,  587-599. 
Bartter,  F.  C.  (1956).  The  role  of  aldosterone  in  normal  homeostatsis  and  in  certain 

disease  states.  Metabolism  Clin,  and  Exptl.  5,  369-383. 
Bartter,  F.  C.  (1957).  The  role  of  aldosterone  in  the  regulation  of  body  fluid  volume  and 

composition.  Scand.  J.  Clin.  &  Lab.  Invest.  10,  50-61. 
Bartter,  F.  C.,  and  Gann,  P.  S.  (1960).  On  the  hemodynamic  regulation  of  the  secretion 

of  aldosterone.  Circulation  21,  1016-1023. 
Bartter,  F.  C.,  Liddle,  G.  W.,  Duncan,  L.  D.,  Jr.,  Barber,  J.  K.,  and  Delea,  C.  (1956). 

The  regulation  of  aldosterone  secretion  in  man:  The  role  of  fluid  volume.  /.  Clin. 

Invest.  35,  1306-1315. 
Bartter,  F.  C.,  Biglieri,  E.  G.,  Pronove,  P.,  and  Delea,  C.  S.  (1958).  Effect  of  changes  in 

intravascular  volume  on  aldosterone  secretion  in  man.  In  "Aldosterone,"  Intern. 

Symposium,  Geneva,  1957  (A.  F.  Muller  and  C.  M.  O'Connor,  eds.),  pp.  100-110. 

Little,  Brown,  Boston,  Massachusetts. 
Bartter,  F.  C,  Mills,  I.  H.,  Biglieri,  E.  G.,  and  Delea,  C.  S.,  (1959).  Studies  on  the 

control  and  physiologic  action  of  aldosterone  secretion.  Recent  Progr.  in  Hormone 

Research,  15,  311-344. 
Bastenie,  P.  A.,  and  Ermans,  A.  jVI.  (1958).  Effect  of  cortisone  on  the  action  of  thyroxine 

in  hypophysectomized  rats.  Endocrinology  62,  245-250. 
Beer,  J.  R.,  and  Mej-er,  R.  K.  (1951).  Seasonal  changes  in  the  endocrine  organs  and 

behavior  patterns  of  the  muskrat.  J.  Mammal.  32,  173-191. 
Bell,  G.  H.,  Davidson,  J.  N.,  and  Scarborough,  H.  (1950).  "Textbook  of  Physiology  and 

Biochemistry."  Williams  &  Wilkins,  Baltimore,  Maryland. 
Benua,  R.  S.,  and  Howard,  E.  (1950).  A  carbonyl  reaction  differentiating  the  fetal  zona 

reticularis  of  the  human  adrenal  cortex  from  the  mouse  X-zone.  Bull.  Johns  Hopkins 

Hasp.  86,  200-213. 
Berson,  S.  A.,  and  Yalow,  R.  S.  (1952).  The  effect  of  cortisone  on  the  iodine  accumulating 

function  of  the  thyroid  gland  in  euthj'roid  subjects.  J.  Clin.  Endocrinol,  and  Meta- 
bolism 12,  407-422. 
Beyler,  A.  L.,  and  Szego,  C.  M.  (1954).  Steroid  hormone  interactions  as  evidenced  by 

modification  of  j8-glucuronidase  activity  of  the  preputial  glands  of  the  rat.  Endo- 
crinology 54,  334-337. 


2.  Endocrines  and  Populations  331 

Bing,  R.  J.  (1952).  The  role  of  coronary  circulation  in  shock.  Ann.  N.  Y.  Acad.  Set. 
55,  367-369. 

Birmingham,  M.  K.,  and  Kurlents,  E.  (1958j.  Inactivation  of  ACTH  by  isolated  rat 
adrenals  and  inhibition  of  corticoid  formation  bj-  adrenocortical  hormones.  Endo- 
crinology 62,  47-60. 

Bloch,  E.,  and  Cohen,  A.  I.  (1960).  In  vitro  steroid  production  by  normal  and  adrenal 
tumor-bearing  mice.  J.  Natl.  Cancer  Inst.  24,  97-107. 

Boas,  N.  F.,  and  Bates,  R.  W.  (1954).  Role  of  the  thyroid  and  anterior  pituitary  glands 
in  the  maintenance  of  the  Harderian  glands  of  the  rat.  Endocrinology  55,  601-612. 

Bogdanove,  E.  M.,  and  Halmi,  N.  S.  (1953).  Effect  of  hypothalamic  lesions  and  sub- 
sequent propylthiouracil  treatment  on  pituitary  structure  in  the  rat.  Endocrinology 
53,  274-292. 

Bogoroch,  R.,  and  Timiras,  P.  (1951).  The  response  of  the  thyroid  gland  of  the  rat  to 
severe  stress.  Endocrinology  49,  548-556. 

Bohanan,  E.  H.  (1939).  Effects  of  environmental  factors  on  the  length  of  estms  cycles 
in  the  rat.  Am.  J.  Hyg.  29A,  1-10. 

Bois,  P.,  and  Selj-e,  H.  (1957).  Enhancement  b}-  thryoxine  of  the  adrenal-stimulating 
effect  of  somatotrophic  hormone.  Endocrinology  15,  171-179. 

Boring,  W.  O.,  Angevine,  D.  M.,  and  Walker,  D.  L.  (1955).  Factors  influencing  host- 
virus  interactions.  I.  A  comparison  of  viral  multiplication  and  histopathology  in 
infant,  adult,  and  cortisone-treated  adult  mice  infected  with  the  Conn  .-5  strain  of 
Coxsackie  virus.  /.  Exptl.  Med.  102,  753-766. 

Boring,  W.  O.,  ZuRhein,  G.  M.,  and  Walker,  D.  L.  (1956).  Factors  influencing  host-virus 
interactions.  II.  Alterations  of  Coxsackie  virus  infection  in  adult  mice  bj'  cold. 
Proc.  Soc.  Exptl.  Biol.  Med.  93,  273-277. 

Botkin,  A.  L.,  and  Tew,  J.  T.  (1952).  Tlu'roid  and  adrenal  respon.se  to  varying  dose 
levels  of  epinephrine.  Proc.  Soc.  Exptl.  Biol.  Med.  79,  378-379. 

Bourliere,  F.  (1951).  "Vie  et  moeurs  des  mammiferes,"  pp.  1-250.  Payot,  Paris. 

Bourne,  G.  H.  (1949).  "The  Mammalian  Adrenal  Gland,"  pp.  1-239.  Clarendon,  Oxford. 

Bradlow,  H.  L.,  and  Gallagher,  T.  F.  (1957).  Metabolism  of  l]-/3-hydroxy-A^-androstene- 
3,17-dione  in  man.  J.  Biol.  Chem.  229,  505-518. 

Bradlow,  H.  L.,  Dobriner,  K.,  and  Gallagher,  T.  F.  (1954).  The  fate  of  cortisone-T  in 
mice.  Endocrinology  54,  343-352. 

Brimblecombe,  R.  W.,  Halkerston,  I.  D.  K.,  and  Reiss,  AI.  (1954).  The  effect  of  various 
ACTH  extracts  on  the  gonads.  /.  Endocrinol.  10,  291-301. 

Brown,  R.  Z.  (1953).  Social  behavior,  reproduction,  and  population  changes  in  the 
house  mouse.  Ecol.  Monographs  23,  217-240. 

Brown-Grant,  K.  (1955).  Inhibition  of  the  release  of  thyroidal  radioiodine  in  the  rat  by 
cortisone.  Endocrinology  56,  607-609. 

Brown-Grant,  K.,  Von  Euler,  C,  Harris,  G.  W.,  and  Reichhn,  S.  (1954a).  The  measure- 
ment and  experimental  modification  of  thyroid  activitj-  in  the  rabbit.  /.  Physiol. 
{London)  126,  1-28. 

Brown-Grant,  K.,  Harris,  G.  W.,  and  Reichlin,  S.  (1954b).  The  effect  of  emotional  and 
physical  stress  on  thjToid  activit}^  in  the  rabbit.  J.  Physiol.  (London)  126,  29-40. 

Brown-Grant,  K.,  Harris,  G.  W.,  and  Reichlin,  S.  (1954c).  The  influence  of  the  adrenal 
cortex  on  thyroid  activity  in  the  rabbit.  J.  Physiol.  (London)  126,  41-51. 

Brown-Grant,  K.,  Harris,  G.  W.,  and  Reichlin,  S.  (1957).  The  effect  of  pituitary-stalk 
section  on  thyroid  function  in  the  rabbit.  J.  Physiol.  [London)  136,  364-379. 

Bruce,  H.  M.,  and  Sloviter,  H.  A.  (1957).  Effect  of  destruction  of  thyroid  tissue  by 
radioactive  iodine  on  reproduction  in  mice.  ./.  Endocrinol.  15,  72-82. 

Bull,  P.  C.  (1955).  Population  regulation  in  rabbit  nematodes.  Natwe  175,  218. 


332  /.  /.  Christian 

Bull,  P.  C.  (1957).  Changing  incidence  of  parasites  in  a  declining  population.  Proc.  New 

Zealand  Ecol.  Soc.  5,  11-12. 
Bull,  P.  C.  (1958).  Incidence  of  Coccidia  (Sporozoa)  in  wild  rabbits  Orydolagus  cuniculus 

(L)  in  Hawke's  Bay,  New  Zealand.  New  Zealand  J.  Set.  1,  289-328. 
BuUough,  W.  S.  (1952).  Stress  and  epidermal  mitotic  activity.  1.  The  effects  of  the 

adrenal  hormones.  /.  Endocrinol.  8,  265-274. 
BuUough,  W.  S.  (1955).  Hormones  and  mitotic  activity.  Vitamins  and  Hormones  13, 

261-292. 
Burn,  J.  H.,  and  Rand,  M.  J.  (1957).  Reserpine  and  adrenaline  in  artery  walls.  Lancet 

273,  1097. 
Burn,  J.  H.,  and  Rand,  M.  J.  (1958a).  Noradrenaline  in  artery  walls  and  its  dispersal. 

Brit.  Med.  J.  I,  903-908. 
Burn,  J.  H.,  and  Rand,  M.  J.  (1958b).  The  action  of  sympathomimetic  amines  in  animals 

treated  with  reserpine.  /.  Physiol.  (London)  144,  314-336. 
Burrows,  H.  (1949).  "Biological  Actions  of  Sex  Hormones,"  2nd  ed.  Cambridge  Univ. 

Press,  London  and  New  York. 
Bush,  I.  E.  (1953).  Species  differences  in  adrenocortical  secretion.  /.  Endocrinol.  9,  95- 

100. 
Byrnes,  W.  W.,  and  Meyer,  R.  K.  (1951).  The  inhibition  of  gonadotropic  hormone 

secretion  by  physiological  doses  of  estrogen.  Endocrinology  48,  133-136. 
Byrnes,  W.  W.,  and  Shipley,  E.  G.   (1950).  Estrogenic  and  gonadotrophic  hormone 

inhibiting  activity  of  some  adrenal  cortical  substances.  Proc.  Soc.  Exptl.  Biol.  Med. 

74,  308-310. 
Cain,  A.  J.,  and  Harrison,  R.  G.  (1950).  Cytological  and  histochemical  variations  in  the 

adrenal  cortex  of  the  albino  rat.  J.  Anat.  84,  Pt.  2,  196-226. 
Calhoun,  J.  B.  (1948).  Mortality  and  movement  of  brown  rats  in  artificially  super- 
saturated populations.  /.  Wildlife  Management  12,  167-171. 
Calhoun,  J.  B.  (1949).  A  method  for  self-control  of  population  growth  among  mammals 

living  in  the  wild.  Science  109,  333-335. 
Calhoun,  J.  B.  (1950).  The  study  of  wild  animals  under  controlled  conditions.  Ann.  N .  Y. 

Acad.  Sci.  51,  1113-1122. 
Cannon,  W.  B.  (1915).  "Bodily  Changes  in  Pain,  Hunger,  Fear  and  Rage."  Appleton, 

New  York. 
Cannon,  W.  B.  (1932).  "The  Wisdom  of  the  Body."  Norton,  New  York. 
Cater,  O.  B.,  and  Stack-Dunne,  M.  P.  (1953).  The  histological  changes  in  the  adrenal  of 

the  hypophysectomized  rat  after  treatment  with  pituitary  preparations.  J.  Pathol. 

Bacterial.  66,  119-133. 
Cater,  O.  B.,  and  Stack-Dunne,  M.  P.   (1955).  The  efi'ects  of  growth  hormone  and 

corticotrophin  upon  the  adrenal  weight  and  adrenocortical  mitotic  activity  in  the 

hypophysectomized  rat.  J.  Endocrinol.  12,  174-184. 
Cavazos,  L.  F.,  and  Melampy,  R.  M.  (1954).  Cytological  effects  of  testosterone  propio- 
nate on  epithelium  of  rat  seminal  vesicles.  Endocrinology  54,  640-648. 
Chang,  M.  C,  and  Pincus,  G.  (1951).  Physiology  of  fertilization  in  mammals.  Physiol. 

Revs.  31,  1-26. 
Chitty,  D.  (1952).  MortaUty  among  voles  (Microtus  agrestis)  at  Lake  Vj^mwy,  Mont- 
gomeryshire in  1936-9.  Phil.  Trans.  Roy.  Soc.  London  B236,  505-552. 
Chitty,  D.  (1954).  Tuberculosis  among  wild  voles  with  a  discussion  of  other  pathological 

conditions  among  certain  mammals  and  birds.  Ecology  35,  227-237. 
Chitty,  D.  (1955).  Adverse  effects  of  population  density  upon  the  viability  of  later 

generations.  In  "The  Numbers  of  Man  and  Animals,"  pp.  57-67.  Oliver  &  Boyd, 

Edinburgh. 


2.  Endocrines  and  Populations  333 

Chitty,  D.  (1957).  Self-regulation  of  numbers  through  changes  in  viability.  Cold  Spring 

Harbor  Symposia  Quant.  Biol.  22,  277-280. 
Chitty,  H.,  and  Austin,  C.  R.  (1957).  Environmental  modification  of  oestrus  in  the  vole. 

Nature  179,  592-593. 
Chitty,  D.,  Chitty,  H.,  Leslie,  P.  H.,  and  Scott,  J.  C.  (1956).  Changes  in  the  relative 

size  of  the  nucleus  in  the  intervertebral  disc  of  stressed  orkney  voles  (Microtus 

orcadensis).  J.  Pathol.  Bacterial  72,  459-470. 
Christian,  J.  J.  (1950a).  A  field  method  of  determining  the  reproduction  status  of  small 

male  mammals.  J .  Mammal.  31,  95-96. 
Christian,  J.  J.  (1950b).  The  adreno-pituitary  system  and  population  cycles  in  mammals. 

J.  Mammal.  31,  247-259. 
Christian,  J.  J.  (1951).  The  mammals  of  the  Mazinaw  lake  region  of  Ontario;  their 

reproduction  and  population  dynamics.  Ann.  Carnegie  Museum  31,  339-386. 
Christian,  J.  J.  (1954).  The  Relation  of  the  Adrenal  Cortex  to  Population  Size  in  Rodents. 

Doctoral  dissertation,  Johns  Hopkins  School  of  Hygiene  and  Public  Health,  Balti- 
more, Maryland. 
Christian,  J.  J.  (1955aj.  Effect  of  population  size  on  the  weights  of  the  reproductive 

organs  of  white  mice.  Am.  J.  Physiol.  181,  477-480. 
Christian,  J.  J.  (1955b).  Effect  of  population  size  on  the  adrenal  glands  and  reproductive 

organs  of  male  mice  in  populations  of  fixed  size.  Am.  J.  Physiol.  182,  292-300. 
Christian,  J.  J.  (1956).  Adrenal  and  reproductive  responses  to  popultaion  size  in  mice 

from  freely  growing  populations.  Ecology  37,  258-273. 
Christian,  J.  J.  (1959a).  Control  of  population  growth  in  rodents  by  interplay  between 

population  density  and  endocrine  physiology.  Wildlife  Disease  1,  1-38. 
Christian,  J.  J.  (1959b).  The  roles  of  endocrine  and  behavioral  factors  in  the  growth  of 

mammalian  populations.  In  "Comparative  Endocrinology,"  Proc.  Columbia  Uni- 
versity SjTnposium,  1958  (A.  Gorbman,  ed.),  pp.  71-97.  Wiley,  New  York. 
Christian,  J.  J.  (1959c).  Adrenocortical,  splenic  and  reproductive  responses  to  inanition 

and  to  grouping.  Endocrinology  65,  189-197. 
Christian,  J.  J.  (1959d).  Lack  of  correlation  between  adrenal  weight  and  injury  from 

fighting  in  grouped  male  albino  mice.  Proc.  Soc.  Exptl.  Biol.  Med.  101,  166-168. 
Christian,  J.  J.  (1960).  Adrenocortical  and  gonadal  responses  of  female  mice  to  increased 

population  density.  Proc.  Soc.  Exptl.  Biol.  Med.  104,  330-332. 
Christian,  J.  J.   (1961).  Phenomena  associated  with  population  density.  Proc.  Natl. 

Acad.  Sci.  U.  S.  47,  428-449. 
Christian  J.  J.  (1963a).  The  pathology  of  overpopulation.  Military  Med.  128,  571-603. 
Christian,  J.  J.   (1963b).  Interrelated  social  and  endocrine  factors  in  populations  of 

rodents.   (Abstract.)  Proc.  16th  Intern.  Congr.  Zool.,  Washington,  D.  C,  3,  8-13. 
Christian,  J.  J.,  and  Davis,  D.  E.  (1955).  Reduction  of  adrenal  weight  in  rodents  by 

reducing  population  size.  Trans.  N.  Am.  Wildlife  Conf.  20,  177-189. 
Christian,  J.  J.,  and  Davis,  D.  E.  (1956).  The  relationship  between  adrenal  weight  and 

population  status  in  Norway  rats.  J.  Mammal.  37,  475-486. 
Christian,  J.  J.,  Flyger,  V.,  and  Davis,  D.  E.  (1960).  Factors  in  mass  mortality  of  a  herd 

of  sika  deer.  Chesapeake  Sci.  1,  79-95. 
Christian,  J.  J.,  and  LeMunyan,  C.  D.  (1958).  Adverse  effects  of  crowding  on  lactation 

and  reproduction  of  mice  and  two  generations  of  their  offspring.  Endocrinology  63, 

517-529. 
Christian,  J.  J.,  and  Ratclifte,  H.  L.  (1952).  Shock  disease  in  captive  wild  mammals. 

Am.  J.  Pathol.  28,  725-737. 
Christian,  J.  J.,  and  Williamson,  H.  O.  (1958).  Effect  of  crowding  on  experimental 

granuloma  formation  in  mice.  Proc.  Soc.  Exptl.  Biol.  Med.  99,  385-387. 


334  J.  J.   Christian 

Christianson,  M.,  and  Jones,  I.  C.  (1957).  The  interrelationships  of  the  adrenal  glands  of 

mother  and  fetus  in  the  rat.  /.  Endocrinol.  15,  17-42. 
Chu,  J.  P.  (1944).  The  influence  of  the  thyroid  on  pregnancy  and  parturition  in  the 

rabbit.  /.  Endocrinol.  4,  109-114. 
Clarke,  J.  R.  (1953).  The  effect  of  fighting  on  the  adrenals,  thymus,  and  spleen  of  the 

vole  {Microtus  agrestis).  J.  Endocrinol.  9,  114-126. 
Clarke,  J.  R.  (1955).  Influence  of  numbers  on  reproduction  and  sui-vival  in  two  experi- 
mental vole  populations.  Proc.  Roy.  Soc.  B144,  6S-S5. 
Clayton,  G.  W.,  Bell,  W.  R.,  and  Guillemin,  R.  (1957).  Stimulation  of  ACTH-release  in 

humans  by  non-pressor  fraction  from  commercial  extracts  of  posterior  pituitarj'. 

Proc.  Soc.  Exptl.  Biol.  Med.  96,  777-779. 
Cole,  D.  F.  (1957).  Effect  of  aldosterone  on  renal  excretion  of  intravenously  administered 

saline.  Endocrinology  60,  562-565. 
Cook,  E.  F.,  and  Beer,  J.  R.  (1958).  A  study  of  louse  populations  on  the  meadow  vole 

and  deer  mouse.  Ecology  39,  645-659. 
Crew,  F.  A.,  and  Mirskaia,  L.  (1931).  The  effects  of  density  on  an  adult  mouse  popu- 
lation. Biol.  Generalis  7,  239-250. 
Critchlow,  B.  \.,  and  Sawjer,  C.  H.  (1955).  Electrical  activity  of  the  rat  brain  cor- 
related with  neurogenic  stimulation  of  the  adenohypophysis.  Federation  Proc.  14, 

32-33. 
Crowcroft,  P.   (1954).  Aggressive  behavior  in   wild   house   mice    {Mus   musculus   L.) 

Ministrj'  of  Agr.  and  Fish  Infestation  Control  Division,  Research  Rept.  No.  35. 
Crowcroft,  P.  and  Rowe,  F.  P.  (1957).  The  growth  of  confined  colonies  of  the  wild  house 

mouse  {Mus  vnisculus  L.).  Proc.  Zool.  Soc.  (London)  129,  359-370. 
Curley,  F.  J.,  and  Ingalls,  T.  H.  (1957).  HjiJoxia  at  normal  atmospheric  pressure  as  a 

cause  of  congenital  malformations  in  mice.  Proc.  Soc.  Exptl.  Biol.  Med.  94,  87-88. 
D'Angelo,   S.   A.    (1955).  Pituitary  regulation  of  thyroid  gland  function.  Brookhaven 

Symposia  in  Biol.  7,  9-29. 
D'Angelo,  S.  A.,  and  Traum,  R.  E.  (1958).  An  experimental  analysis  of  the  hypothalamic 

hypophyseal-thyroid  relationship  in  the  rat.  Ann.  N.  Y.  Acad.  Sci.  72,  239-270. 
D'Angelo,  S.  A.,  Gordon,  A.  S.,  and  Charipper,  H.  A.  (1948).  A  differential  response  of 

the  rodent  adrenal  gland  to  acute  starvation.  Proc.  Soc.  Exptl.  Biol.  Med.  68,  527- 

531. 
Davidson,  C.  S.  (1937).  Effect  of  adrenotropic  extract  upon  the  accessory  reproductive 

organs  of  castrated  rats.  Proc.  Soc.  Exptl.  Biol.  Med.  36,  703-705. 
Davidson,  C.  S.,  and  Moon,  H.  D.   (1936).  Effect  of  adrenocorticotropic  extracts  on 

accessory  reproductive  organs  in  castrate  rats.  Proc.  Soc.  Exptl.  Biol.  Med.  35, 

281-282. 
Davis,  D.  E.  (1951a).  The  relation  between  level  of  population  and  pregnancy  of  Noiwaj- 

rate.  Ecology  32,  459-461. 
Davis,  D.  E.   (1951b).  The  relation  between  level  of  population  and  size  and  sex  of 

Norway  rats.  Ecology  32,  462-464. 
Davis,  D.  E.  (1951c).  The  relation  between  the  level  of  population  and  the  prevalence  of 

Leptospira,  Salmonella,  and  capillaria  in  Norway  rats.  Ecology  32,  465-468. 
Davis,  D.  E.  (1953).  The  characteristics  of  rat  populations.  Quart.  Rev.  Biol.  28,  373-401. 
Davis,  D.  E.,  and  Christian,  J.  J.  (1957).  Relations  of  adrenal  weight  to  social  rank  of 

mice.  Proc.  Soc.  Exptl.  Biol.  Med.  94,  728-731. 
Davis,  D.  E.,  and  Christian,  J.  J.  (1958).  Population  consequences  of  a  sustained  j-ield 

program  for  Norwaj'  rats.  Ecology  39,  217-222. 
Davis,  D.  E.,  and  Jensen,  W.  L.  (1952).  Mortality  in  an  induced  epidemic.  Trans.  N. 

Am.  Wildlife  Conf.  17,  151-160. 


2.  Endocrines  and  Populations  335 

Davis,  D.  E.,  and  Prudovsky,  S.  (.1959).  Effect  of  behavior  on  d<'veloi)ment  of  resistance 

to  tetanus  toxin.  Unpublished. 
Davis,  D.  E.,  and  Read,  C.  P.  (1958).  Effect  of  behavior  on  development  of  resistance  in 

trichinosis.  Proc.  Soc.  Exptl.  Biol.  Med.  99,  269-272. 
Davis,  J.  O.  (1963)  The  role  of  the  adrenal  corte.x  and  the  kidney  in  the  pathogenesis  of 

cardiac  edema.  Yale  J.  Biol.  Med.  35,  402-428. 
Davis,  J.  O.,  Goodkind,  M.  J.,  Pechet,  M.  M.,  and  Ball,  W.  C,  Jr.  (1956).  Increased 

excretion  of  aldosterone  in  urine  from  dogs  with  right-sided  congestive  heart  failure 

and  from  dogs  with  thoracic  inferior  vena  cava  constriction.  Am.  J.  Phyniol.  187, 

45-50. 
Davis,  J.  O.,  Pechet,  M.  M.,  Ball,  W.  C,  Jr.,  and  Goodkind,  M.  J.  (1957).  Increased 

aldosterone  secretion  in  dogs  with  rightsided  congestive  heart  failure  and  in  dogs 

with  thoracic  inferior  vena  cava  constriction.  /.  Clin.  Invest.  36,  689. 
Davis,  J.  O.,  Kliman,  B.,  Yankopoulos,  N.  A.,  Peterson,  R.  E.,  and  Casper,  A.  (1958). 

Increased  aldosterone  secretion  following  acute  constriction  of  the  inferior  vena 

cava.  /.  Clin.  Invest.  37,  1783-1790. 
Davis,  J.  O.,  Bahn,  R.  C,  Yankopoulos,  N.  A.,  KHman,  B.,  and  Peterson,  R.  E.  (19.59a). 

Acute  effects  of  hypophysectomy  and  diencephalic  lesions  on  aldosterone  secretion. 

Am.  J.  Physiol.  197,  380-386. 
Davis,  J.  O.,  Yankopoulos,  N.  A.,  and  Holraan,  J.  (1959b).  Chronic  effects  of  carotid 

sinus  denervation  cervical  vagotomy  and  aortic  depressor  nerve  section  on  aldo- 

.sterone  and  sodium  excretion.  Am.  J.  Physiol.  197,  207-210. 
Davis,  M.  E.,  and  Plotz,  E.  J.  (1954).  The  effects  of  cortisone  acetate  on  intact  and 

adrenalectomized  rats  during  pregnancy.  Endocrinology  54,  384-395. 
Dawson,  J.  (1956).  Splenic  hj-pertrophy  in  voles.  Nature  178,  1183-1184. 
Deane,  H.  W.,  and  Morse,  A.  (1948).  The  cytological  distribution  of  ascorbic  acid  in  the 

adrenal  cortex  of  the  rat  under  normal  and  experimental  conditions.  Anat.  Record 

100,  127-141. 
Deane,  H.  W.,  and  Seligman,  A.  M.  (1953).  Evaluation  of  procedures  for  the  cyto- 
logical localization  of  ketosteroids.  Vitamins  and  Hormones  11,  173-204. 
Deane,  H.  W.,  Shaw,  J.  H.,  and  Creep,  R.  O.  (1948).  The  effect  of  altered  sodium  of 

potassium  intake  on  the  width  and  cytochemistry  of  the  zona  glomerulosa  of  the 

rat's  adrenal  cortes.  Endocrinology  43,  133-153. 
Deanesly,  R.  (1928).  A  study  of  the  adrenal  cortex  in  the  mouse  and  its  relation  to  the 

gonads.  Proc.  Roy.  Soc.  B103,  523-546. 
DeBodo,  R.  C,  and  Altzuler,  N.   (1958).  Insulin  hypersensitivity  and  physiological 

insulin  antagonists.  Physiol.  Revs.  38,  389-445. 
Delost,  P.  (1951).  Structure  histologiciue  et  etude  I'activite  phosphatasique  alcahne  du 

canal  deferent  du  campagnol  des  champs    (Microtus  arvalis,  P.)   etat  du  cortex 

surrenal.  Compt.  rend.  soc.  biol.  145,  373-377. 
Delost,  P.  (1952a).  Le  cortex  surrenal  du  campagnol  des  champs  {Microtus  arvalis,  P.) 

et  ses  modifications  aprcs  castration.  Compt.  rend.  soc.  biol.  146,  27-31. 
Delost,  P.  (1952b).  Modifications  histochimiques  du  cortex  surrenal  du  campagnol  des 

champs  {Microtus  arvalis,  P.)  apres  castration.  Compt.  rend.  soc.  biol.  146,  1080-1083. 
Delost,  P.  (1954).  Action  involutive  de  la  cortisone  sur  la  zone  X  surrenalienne  du  cam- 
pagnol des  champs  {Microtus  arvalis.  P.).  Compt.  rend.  soc.  biol.  148,  1161-1163. 
Delost,  P.  (1955).  Role  de  I'ovaire  et  du  cortex  surrenal  dans  la  stimulation  de  la  prostate 

femelle  chez  les  rongeurs  sauvages.  Compt.  rend.  soc.  biol.  149,  1336-1339. 
Delost,  P.  (1956a).  Les  correlations  genito-surrenaliennes  chez  le  campagnol  des  champs 

{Microtus  arvalis,  P.).  Ann.  Sci.  nat.  Zool.  11.  18,  392-567. 


336  J.  J.  Christian 

Delost,  P.  (1956b).  Donnees  experimentales  comparees  sur  la  zone  X  surrenalienne. 

Action  de  la  cortisone  et  de  la  testosterone.  Compt.  rend.  soc.  biol.  150,  1330-13332. 
Delost,  P.  (1957).  Recherches  sur  le  cortex  surrenal  et  la  zone  X  dcs  musaraignes  {Sorex 

araneus  L.  et  Crocidura  russida  H.).  Compt.  rend.  soc.  biol.  151,  484-487. 
Delost,  P.,  and  Delost,  H.  (1954).  Existence  d'une  zone  X  surrenalienne  chez  le  cam- 

pagnol  roussatre  {Clethrionomys  glareolus,  S.).  Compt.  rend.  soc.  biol.  148,  1788-1793. 
Delost,  P.,  and  Delost,  H.  (1955).  Modifications  ponderales  et  histologiques  des  glandes 

surrenales  du  campagnol  agreste  {Microtus  agrestis,  L.)  en  fonction  de  I'etat  sexual. 

Compt.  rend.  soc.  biol.  149,  910-914. 
Dempsey,  E.  W.  (1948).  The  chemical  cytolog}-  of  endocrine  glands.  Recent  Progr.  in 

Hormone  Research  3,  127-157. 
Dempsey,  E.  W.,  and  Uotila,  U.  U.  (1940).  The  effect  of  pituitary  stalk  section  upon 

reproductive  phenomena  in  the  female  rat.  Endocrinology  27,  573-579. 
Denison,  M.  E.,  and  Zarrow,  M.  X.  (1955).  Changes  in  estrous  cycle  of  rat  during  pro- 
longed exposure  to  cold.  Proc.  Soc.  Exptl.  Biol.  Med.  89,  632-634. 
Desclin,  J.  (1959).  Masculinisation  spontanee  et  experimentale  des  glandes  sous-maxil- 

laires  chez  la  souris  femelle.  Compt.  rend.  acad.  sci.  248,  597-600. 
Dorfman,  A.  (1953).  The  effects  of  adrenal  hormones  on  connective  tissues.  Ann.  N.  Y. 

Acad.  Sci.  56,  698-703. 
Dorfman,  R.  I.  (1949).  The  bioassay  of  adrenocortical  steroids.  Ann.  N.  Y.  Acad.  Sci. 

50,  556-575. 
Dorfman,  R.  I.  (1959).  Comparative  biochemistry  of  the  adrenocortical  hormones.  In 

"Comparative    Endocrinology,"    Proc.    Columbia    University    Symposium,    1958 

(A.  Gorbman,  ed.),  pp.  613-623.  Wiley,  New  York. 
Dorfman,  R.  I.  (1960).  Steroid  metabolism  in  endocrine  tumors.  In  "Biological  Activ- 
ities of  Steroids  in  Relation  to  Cancer"  (G.  Pincus  and  E.  P.  VoUmer,  eds.),  pp.  93- 

110.  Academic  Press,  New  York. 
Dorfman,  R.  I.,  and  Shipley,  R.  A.  (1956).  "Androgens.  Biochemistry  Physiology  and 

Clinical  Significance."  Wiley,  New  York. 
Dougherty,   T.  F.    (1952a).   Studies  of  the  antiphlogistic  and  antibody  suppressing 

functions  of  the  pituitary  adrenocortical  secretions.   Recent  Progr.  in  Hormone 

Research  7,  307-330. 
Dougherty,  T.  F.  (1952b).  Effect  of  hormones  on  Ij-mphatic  tissue.  Physiol.  Revs.  32, 

379-401. 
Dougherty,  T.  F.  (1953).  Some  observations  on  mechanisms  of  corticosteroid  action  on 

inflamnation  and  immunologic  processes.  Ann.  N.  Y.  Acad.  Sci.  56,  748-756. 
Dougherty,  T.  F.,  and  Schneebeli,  G.  L.  (1955).  The  use  of  steroids  as  anti-inflammatory 

agents.  Ann.  N.  Y.  Acad.  Sci.  61,  328-348. 
Drager,  G.  A.   (1950).  Neurosecretion  following  hypophysectomy.  Proc.  Soc.  Exptl. 

Biol.  Med.  75,  712-713. 
Ehrich,  W.  E.,  and  Seifter,  J.  (1948).  Role  played  by  salivary  glands  in  the  alarm  re- 
reaction.  A.  M.  A.  Arch.  Pathol.  45,  239-245. 
Eik-nes,  K.  (1959).  Comments  on  a  paper  by  J.  W.  Mason.  Recent  Progr.  in  Hormone 

Research  15,  380-381. 
Eisenstein,   A.   B.    (1957).   Pantothenic   acid   and  adrenocortical   hormone   secretion. 

Endocrinology  60,  298-302. 
Eisenstein,  A.  B.  (1959).  Relationship  of  vitamin  Be  to  adrenocortical  function  in  the 

rat.  Proc.  Soc.  Exptl.  Biol.  Med.  100,  111-114. 
Eisenstein,  A.  B.,  and  Hartroft,  P.  M.  (1957).  Alterations  in  the  rat  adrenal  cortex 

induced  by  sodium  deficiency:  Steroid  hormone  secretion.  Endocrinology  60,  634-640. 


2.  Endocrines  and  Populations  337 

Elias,  H.  (1948).  Growth  of  the  adrenal  cortex  in  domesticated  Ungulata.  Am.  J.  Vet. 

Research  9,  173-189. 
Ehas,  H.,  and  Pauly,  J.  E.  (1956).  The  structure  of  the  human  adrenal  cortex.  Endo- 
crinology 58,  714-738. 
Elmadjian,  F.,  Hope,  J.  M.,  and  Lamson,  E.  T.  (1958).  Excretion  of  epinephrine  and 

norepinephrine  under  stress.  Recent  Progr.  in  Hormone  Research  14,  513-553. 
Elton,  C.  (1942).  "Voles,  Mice  and  Lemmings."  Clarendon,  Oxford. 
Engel,  F.  L.  (1952).  The  significance  of  the  metabolic  changes  during  shock.  Ann.  N.  Y. 

Acad.  Sci.  55,  381-393. 
Eranko,  O.,  and  Hopsu,  V.  (1958).  Effect  of  reserpine  on  the  histochemistry  and  content 

of  adrenaline  and  noradrenaline  in  the  adrenal  medulla  of  the  rat  and  the  mouse. 

Endocrinology  62,  15-23. 
Eranko,  O.,  and  Raisanen,  L.   (1957).  Adrenaline  and  noradrenaline  in  the  adrenal 

medulla  during  postnatal  development  of  the  rat.  Endocrinology  60,  753-760. 
Eranko,  O.,  Hopsu,  V.,  and  Raisanen,  L.  (1959).  The  effect  of  prolonged  administrations 

of  nicotine  on  the  medullary  volume  and  the  distribution  of  noradrenaline  in  the 

adrenals  of  the  rat,  the  mouse,  and  the  guinea  pig.  Endocrinology  65,  293-297. 
Erickson,  A.  B.  (1944).  Helminth  infections  in  relation  to  population  fluctuations  in 

snowshoe  hares.  J.  Wildlife  Management  8,  134-153. 
Errington,  P.  L.  (1943).  An  analysis  of  mink  predation  upon  muskrats  in  North-central 

United  States.  Iowa  State  College  Agr.  Expt.  Sta.  Bidl.  No.  320. 
Errington,  P.  L.  (1948).  Ecology  of  the  muskrat.  Report  on  Agr.  Research  for  year-ending 

June  30,  1948.  Iowa  State  College  Agr.  Expt.  Sta.  pp.  230-232. 
Errington,  P.  L.   (1951).  Concerning  fluctuations  in  populations  of  the  prolific  and 

widely  distributed  muskrat.  Am.  Naturalist  85,  273-292. 
Errington,  P.  L.  (1954a).  On  the  hazards  of  overemphasizing  numerical  fluctuations  in 

studies  of  "cyclic"  phenomena  in  muskrat  populations.  /.  Wildlife  Management  18, 

66-90. 
Errington,  P.  L.  (1954b).  The  special  responsiveness  of  minks  to  epizootics  in  muskrat 

populations.  Ecol.  Monographs  24,  377-393. 
Errington,  P.  L.   (1957).  Of  populations,  cycles  and  unknowns.   Cold  Spring  Harbor 

Symposia  Quant.  Biol.  22,  287-300. 
Ershoff,  B.  H.  (1948).  Effects  of  thiouracil  feeding  on  resistance  to  low  environmental 

temperature.  Endocrinology  43,  36-39. 
Evans,  E.  S.,  Simpson,  M.  E.,  and  Evans,  H.  M.  (1958).  The  role  of  growth  hormone  in 

calorigenesis  and  thjToid  function.  Endocrinology  63,  836-852. 
Everett,  J.  W.  (1956).  Functional  corpora  lutea  maintained  for  months  by  autografts  of 

rat  hypophyses.  Endocrinology  58,  786-796. 
Everett,  J.  W.  (1959).  Neuroendocrine  mechanisms  in  control  of  the  mammalian  ovary. 

In  "Comparative  Endocrinology  Proc.  Columbia  University  Symposium,"  1958 

(A.  Gorbman,  ed.),  pp.  74-186.  Wiley,  New  York. 
Everett,  J.  W.,  and  Sawyer,  C.  H.   (1953).   Estimated  duration  of  the  spontaneous 

activation  which  causes  release  of  ovulation  hormone  from  the  rat  hypophysis. 

Endocrinology  52,  83-92. 
Farrell,  G.  L.  (1958).  Regulation  of  aldosterone  secretion.  Physiol.  Revs.  38,  709-728. 
Farrell,  G.  L.  (1959a).  The  physiological  factors  which  influence  the  secretion  of  aldo-' 

sterone.  Recent  Progr.  in  Hormone  Research  15,  275-310. 
Farrell,  G.  L.  (1959b).  Steroidogenic  properties  of  extracts  of  beef  diencephalon.  Endo- 
crinology 65,  29-33. 


338  J.  J.   Christian 

Farrell,  G.  L.,  and  Laqueur,  G.  (1955).  Reduction  of  pituitary  content  of  ACTH  by 
cortisone.  Endocrinology  56,  471-473. 

Farrell,  G.  L.,  Rauschkolb,  E.  W.,  and  Royce,  P.  C.  (1955).  Secretion  of  aldosterone  by 
the  adrenal  of  the  dog.  Effects  of  hypophysectomy  and  ACTH.  Am.  J.  Physiol.  182, 
269-272. 

Farrell,  G.  L.,  Fleming,  R.  B.,  Rauschkolb,  E.  W.,  Yat.su,  F.  M.,  McCally,  M.,  and 
Anderson,  C.  H.  (1958).  Steroidogenic  properties  of  purified  corticotropins.  Endo- 
crinology 62,  506-512. 

Feldman,  J.  D.  (1950).  Histochemical  reactions  of  adrenal  cortical  cells.  Anat.  Record 
107,  347-358. 

Feldman,  J.  D.  (1951).  Endocrine  control  of  the  adrenal  gland.  Anat.  Record  109,  41-69. 

Fitch,  H.  S.  (1957).  Aspects  of  reproduction  and  develoi)ment  in  the  prairie  vole  {Micro- 
tus  ochrogaster) .  Univ.  Kansas  Museum  Nat.  Hist.  Publ.  10,  129-161. 

Flexner,  L.  B.,  and  Grollman,  A.  (1939).  The  reduction  of  osmic  acid  as  an  indicator  of 
adrenal  cortical  activity  in  the  rat.  Anat.  Record  75,  207-221. 

Flick,  S.,  Davis,  D.  E.,  and  Christian,  J.  J.  (1959).  Effect  of  constant  length  of  day  on 
reproduction  in  albino  mice.  Brit.  J.  Hyg.  57,  193-197. 

Fliickiger,  E.,  and  VerZc4r,  F.  (1955).  Thyroid  function  after  adrenalectomy.  J.  En- 
docrinol. 13, 39-45. 

Folley,  S.  J.  (1956).  "The  Physiology  and  Biochemistry  of  Lactation."  C.  C.  Thomas, 
Springfield,  Illinois. 

Fortier,  C.  (1956).  Hypothalamic  control  of  the  adrencorticotrophic  function  SOth 
Intern.  Congr.  Physiol.,  Brussels,  Abstr.  of  Revs.  1-18. 

Fortier,  C.  (1957).  Neuroendocrine  relationships.  Progr.  in  Kexirol.  and  Psychiat.  12, 
252-261. 

Fortier,  C.  (1959a).  Pituitary  ACTH  and  plasma  free  corticosteroids  following  bilateral 
adrenalectomy  in  the  rat.  Proc.  Sac.  Exptl.  Biol.  Med.  100,  13-16. 

Fortier,  C.  (1959b).  Effect  of  hydrocortisone  on  pituitary  ACTH  and  adrenal  weight  in 
the  rat.  Proc.  Sac.  Exptl.  Biol.  Med.  100,  16-19. 

Frank,  F.  (1953).  Untersuchungen  liber  den  Zusammenbruch  von  Feldmausplagen 
{Microtus  arvalis  Pallas).  Zool.  Jahrh.  Jena.  82,  95-136. 

Fraser,  F.  C,  Fainstat,  T.  D.,  and  Kalter,  H.  (1953).  The  experimental  production  of 
congenital  defects  with  particular  reference  to  cleft  palate.  Neo-Natal  Studies  2, 
43-58. 

Fredrickson,  D.  S.,  and  (iordon,  R.  S.,  Jr.  (1958).  Tran.sport  of  fatty  acids.  Physiol- 
Revs.  38,  585-630. 

Freedman,  H.  H.,  and  (cordon,  A.  S.  (1955).  Endocrine  factors  in  response  of  the  hypo- 
thyroid rat  to  cold.  Endocrinology  56,  135-142. 

Gaddum,  J.  H.,  and  Holzbauer,  M.  (1957).  Adrenaline  and  noradrenaline.  Vitamins  and 
Hormones  15,  152-205. 

Gallagher,  T.  F.  (1958).  Steroid  hormone  metabolism  and  the  control  of  adrenal  secretion. 
Harvey  Lectures  Ser.  52,  1-22. 

Gallagher,  T.  F.,  Kappas,  A.,  Hellman,  L.,  Lipsett,  M.  B.,  Pearson,  O.  H.,  and  West, 
C.  D.  (1958).  Adrenocortical  hjperfunction  in  "idiopathic"  hirsutism  and  the 
Stein-Leventhal  syndrome.  Abstr.  Endocrine  Soc.  Program,  p.  15. 

Ganong,  W.  F.  (1959).  Adrenal-hypoph3^seal  interrelations.  In  "Comparative  Endo- 
crinolog}^,"  Proc.  Columbia  University  Symposium,  1958  (A.  Gorbman,  ed.), 
pp.  187-201. 

Gaunt,  R.  Renzi,  A.  A.,  and  Chart,  J.  J.  (1955).  Aldosterone:  a  review.  J.  Clin.  Endo- 
crinol, and  Metabolism  15,  621-646. 


2.  Endocrines  and  Populations  339 

Gersh,  I.,  and  Grollman,  A.  (1939).  The  nature  of  the  X-zone  of  the  adrenal  gland  of 

the  mouse.  Anat.  Record  75,  131-154. 
Gersh,  I.,  and  Grollman,  A.  (19-41).  The  vascular  pattern  of  the  adrenal  gland  of  the 

mouse  and  rat  and  its  physiological  response  to  changes  in  glandular  activity. 

Carnegie  Inst.  Wash.  Publ.  No.  525,  111-126. 
Gerwing,  J.  (1958).  The  effect  of  continued  toxic  stress  on  the  activity  of  the  thyroid 

gland  in  the  rat  and  the  guinea-pig.  J.  Physiol.  (London)  144,  243-249. 
Gerwing,  J.,  Long,  D.  A.,  and  Pitt-Rivers,  R.  (1958).  The  influence  of  bacterial  exotoxins 

on  the  activity  of  the  thyroid  gland  in  different  species.  J.  Physiol.  [London)  144, 

229-242. 
Giroud,  C.  J.  P.,  Stachenko,  J.,  and  Piletta,  P.  (1958).  Ln  vitro  studies  of  the  functional 

zonation  of  the  adrenal  cortex  and  of  the  production  of  aldosterone.  In  "Aldro- 

sterone,"  Intern.  Symposium,  Geneva,  1957  (A.  F.  Muller  and  C.  M.  O'Connor, 

eds.),  pp.  .56-72,  Little,  Brown,  Boston,  Massachusetts. 
Giroud,  C.  J.  P.,  Stachenko,  J.,  and  Venning,  E.  H.  (19.56).  Secretion  of  aldosterone  by 

the  zona  glomerulosa  of  rat  adrenal  glands  incubated  in  vitro.  Proc.  Soc.  Exptl. 

Biol.  Med.  92,  154-1.58. 
Glaubach,  S.  (1952).  Effect  of  excessive  doses  of  cortisone,  ACTH  and  prolactin  in 

pregnant  and  nursing  mice.  /.  Mt.  Sinai  Hosp.  N.  Y.  19,  84-90. 
Ghck,  D.,  and  Ochs,  M.  J.  (1955).  Studies  in  histochemistry:  Quantitative  histological 

distribution  of  cholesterol  in  adrenal  glands  of  the  cow,  rat  and  monkey,  and  effects 

of  stress  conditions,  ACTH,  cortisone  and  desoxvcorticosterone.  Endocrinology  56, 

285-298. 
Godfrey,  G.  K.  (1955).  Observations  on  the  nature  of  the  decline  in  numbers  of  two 

Microtus  populations.  /.  Mammal.  36,  209-214. 
Goldberg,  R.  C,  Wolff,  J.,  and  Creep,  R.  O.  (1957).  Studies  on  the  nature  of  the  thyroid- 
pituitary  interrelationship.  Endocrinology  60,  3S-52. 
Goldfien,  A.,  Zileh,  M.  S.,  Despointes,  R.  H.,  and  Bethune,  J.  E.  (19.58).  The  effect  of 

hypoglycemia  on  the  adrenal  secretion  of  epinephrine  and  norepinephrine  in  the 

dog.  Endocrinology  62,  749-757. 
Goodman,  H.  AL,  and  Knobil,  E.  (1959).  Mobilization  of  fatty  acids  by  epinephrine  in 

normal  and  hj-pophysectomized  rhesus  monkeys.  Proc.  Soc.  Exptl.  Biol.  Med.  100, 

195-197. 
Gordon,  A.  S.  (1955).  Some  aspects  of  hormonal  influences  upon  the  leukocytes.  Ann. 

N.  Y.  Acad.  Sci.  59,  907-927. 
Gordon,  A.  S.,  and  Katsh,  (J.  F.  (1949).  The  relation  of  the  adrenal  cortex  to  the  struc- 
ture and  phagocytic  activity  of  the  macrophagic  system.  Ann.  N.  Y.  Acad.  Sci. 

52,  1-30. 
Grad,  B.,  and  Leblond,  C.  P.  (1949).  The  necessity  of  testis  and  thyroid  hormones  for 

the  maintenance  of  the  serous  tubules  of  the  submaxillary  glands  in  the  male  rat. 

Endocrinology  45,  250-266. 
Gray,  I.,  and  Beetham,  W.  P.,  Jr.  (1957).  Changes  in  plasma  concentration  of  epine- 
phrine and  norepinephrine  with  muscular  work.  Proc.  Soc.  Exptl.  Biol.  Med.  96, 

636-638. 
Gray,  S.  J.,  and  Ramey,  C.  G.  (1957).  Adrenal  influence  upon  the  stomach  and  the 

gastric  responses  to  stress.  Recent  Progr.  in  Hormone  Research  13,  583-617. 
Green,  J.  D.  (1951).  The  comparative  anatomy  of  the  hypophysis,  with  special  reference 

to  its  blood  supply  and  innervation.  Am.  J.  Anat.  88,  22.5-312. 
Green,  R.  G.,  and  Evans,  C.  A.  (1940a).  Studies  on  a  population  of  snowshoe  hares  on 

the  Lake  Alexander  area.  I.  Gross  annual  censuses  1932-1939.  J.  Wildlife  Manage- 
ment 4,  220-238. 


340  /.  /.  Christian 

Green,  R.  G.,  and  Evans,  C.  A.  (1940b).  Studies  on  a  population  of  snowshoe  hares  on 

the  Lake  Alexander  area.  II.  Mortalitj^  according  to  age  groups  and  seasons.  J. 

Wildlife  Management  4,  267-278. 
Green,  R.  G.,  and  Evans,  C.  A.  (1940c).  Studies  on  a  population  of  snowshoe  hares  on 

the  Lake  Alexander  area.  III.  Effect  of  reproduction  and  mortality  of  young  hares 

on  the  cycle.  /.  Wildlife  Management  4,  347-358. 
Green,  R.  G.,  and  Larson,  C.  L.  (1938).  A  description  of  shock  disease  in  the  snowshoe 

hare.  A)n.  J.  Hyg.  28,  190-212. 
Green,  R.  G.,  Larson,  C.  L.,  and  Bell,  J.  F.  (1939).  Shock  disease  as  the  cause  of  the 

periodic  decimination  of  the  snowshoe  hare.  Am.  J.  Hyg.  SOB,  83-102. 
Greene,  R.  R.,  Burrill,  M.  W.,  and  Ivy,  A.  C.  (1941).  Experimental  intersexuahty:  The 

effects  of  combined  estrogens  and  androgens  on  the  embryonic  sexual  development 

of  the  rat.  /.  Exptl.  Zool.  87,  211-232. 
Greep,  R.  O.,  and  Deane,  H.  W.  (1947).  Cytochemical  evidence  fo  the  cessation  of 

hormone  production  in  the  zona  glomerulosa  of  the  rat's  adrenal  cortex  after  pro- 
longed treatment  with  desoxycorticosterone  acetate.  Endocrinology  40,  417-425. 
Greep,  R.  O.,  and  Deane,  H.  W.  (1949a).  Histological,  cytochemical  and  physiological 

observations  on  the  regeneration  of  the  rat's  adrenal  gland  following  enucleation. 

Endocrinology  45,  42-56. 
Greep,  R.  O.,  and  Deane,  H.  W.  (1949b).  The  cj^tology  and  cytochemistry  of  the  adrenal 

cortex.  Ann.  N.  Y.  Acad.  Sci.  50,  596-615. 
Greer,  M.  A.  (1951).  Evidence  of  hypothalamic  control  of  the  pituitar}^  release  of  thrj^o- 

trophin.  Proc.  Soc.  Exptl.  Biol.  Med.  77,  603-608. 
Greer,  M.  A.  (1952).  The  role  of  the  hypothalamus  in  the  control  of  thyroid  function. 

/.  Clin.  Endocrinol,  and  Metabolism  12,  1259-1268. 
Greer,  M.  A.  (1957).  Studies  on  the  influence  of  the  central  nervous  system  on  anterior 

pituitary  function.  Recent  Progr.  in  Hormone  Research  13,  67-104. 
Gross,  F.,  and  Lichtlen,  F.  (1958).  Further  evidence  for  a  qualitative  difference  between 

aldosterone  and  cortexone.  In  "Aldosterone,"  Intern.  Symposium,  Geneva,  1957 

(A.  F.  MuUer  and  C.  M.  O'Connor,  eds.),  pp.  39-55.  Little,  Brown,  Boston,  Massa- 
chusetts. 
Gross,  J.  (1955).  The  distribution  of  radioactive  th^-roid  hormone  in  tissues.  Brookhaven 

Symposia  in  Biol.  No.  7,  102-110. 
Grosvenor,  C.  E.,  and  Turner,  C.  W.  (1959a).  Effect  of  growth  hormone  and  oxytocin 

upon  milk  yield  in  the  lactating  rat.  Proc.  Soc.  Exptl.  Biol.  Med.  100,  158-161. 
Grosvenor,  C.  E.,  and  Turner,  C,  W.  (1959b).  Effect  of  growth  hormone  upon  thj'roid 

secretion  rate  in  the  rat.  Proc.  Soc.  Exptl.  Biol.  Med.  100,  70-72. 
Grosvenor,  C.  E.,  and  Turner,  C.  W.  (1959c).  Thj-roid  hormone  and  lactation  in  the  rat. 

Proc.  Soc.  Exptl.  Biol.  Med.  100,  162-165. 
Guillemin,  R.  (1955).  A  re-evaluation  of  acetylcholine,  adrenaline,  noradrenaline  and 

histamine  as  possible  mediators  of  the  pituitary  adrenocorticotorphic  activation  by 

stress.  Endocrinology  56,  248-255. 
Guillemin,  R.,  Hearn,  W.  R.,  Cheek,  W.  R.,  and  Housholder,  D.  E.  (1957).  Control  of 

corticotrophin  release:  Further  studies  with  in  vitro  methods.  Endocrinology  60, 

488-506. 
Guillemin,  R.,  Clayton,  G.  W.,  Smith,  J.  D.,  and  Lipscomb,  H.  S.  (1958).  Measurement 

of  free  corticosteroids  in  rat  plasma:  Physiological  validation  of  a  method.  Endo- 
crinology 63,  349-358. 
Hagen,  P.,  and  Welch,  A.  D.  (1956).  The  adrenal  medulla  and  the  biosynthesis  of  pressor 

amines.  Recent  Progr.  in  Hormone  Research  12,  27-44. 


2.  Endocrines  and  Populations  341 

Halmi,  N.  S.,  Spirtos,  B.  N.,  Bogdanove,  E.  M.  and  Lipner,  H.  J.  (1953).  A  study  of 

various  influences  on  the  iodide  concentrating  mechanism  of  the  rat  thryoid.  Endo- 
crinology 52,  19-32. 
Hamilton,  W.  J.,  Jr.  (1937).  The  biology  of  microtine  cycles.  /.  Agr.  Research  54,  779- 

790. 
Hammond,  J.,  Jr.  (1954).  Light  regulation  of  hormone  secretion.  Vitamins  and  Hormones 

12,  157-206. 
Hamolsk}-,  M.  W.,  Gierlach,  Z.  S.,  and  Jensen,  H.  (1951).  Uptake  and  conversion  of 

radioactive  iodine  (P^^)  b}^  the  thyroid  gland  in  vivo  and  in  vitro  in  tourniquet  shock 

in  rats.  Am.  J.  Physiol.  164,  35-53. 
Harris,  G.  W.  (1955a).  The  reciprocal  relationship  between  the  thyroid  and  adreno- 
cortical responses  to  stress.  CIBA  Foundation  Colloq.  on  Endocrinol.  8,  531-550. 
Harris,  G.  W.  (1955b).  "Neural  Control  of  the  Pituitary  Gland."  Arnold,  London. 
Harris,  G.  W.  (1959).  Neuroendocrine  control  of  TSH  regulation.  In  "Comparative 

Endocrinology."  Proc.  Columbia  University  Symposium,  1958  (A.  Gorbman,  ed.), 

pp.  202-222.  Wiley,  New  York. 
Harris,  G.  W.,  and  Fortier,  C.  (1954).  The  regulation  of  anterior  function,  with  special 

reference  to  the  secretion  of  adrenocorticotrophic  hormone.  In  "4th  Annual  Review 

of  Stress"  (H.  Selye  and  G.  Heuser,  eds.),  pp.  106-127.  Acta,  Montreal. 
Harris,  G.  W.,  and  Woods,  J.  W.  (1958).  The  effect  of  electrical  stimulation  of  the 

hypothalamus  or  pituitary  gland  on  thyroid  activity.  /.  Physiol.  (London)  143, 

246-274. 
Harrison,  R.  G.  (1957).  The  adrenal  circulation  in  the  rabbit.  J.  Endocrinol.  15,  64-71. 
Harrison,  R.  G.  (1951).  A  comparative  study  of  the  vascularization  of  the  adrenal  gland 

in  the  rabbit,  rat,  and  cat.  /.  Anat.  85,  12-23. 
Harrison,  R.  G.,  and  Asling,  C.  W.  (1955).  The  anatomj^  and  functional  significance  of 

the  vascularization  of  the  adrenal  gland  in  the  rhesus  monkey  {Macaca  mulatta). 

J.  Anat.  89,  106-113. 
Hart,  P.,  and  Rees,  R.  J.  W.  (1950).  Enhancing  effect  of  cortisone  on  tuberculosis  in 

the  mouse.  Lancet  ii,  391-395. 
Hartman,  F.  A.,  and  Brownell,  K.  A.  (1949).  "The  Adrenal  Glands."  Lea  &  Febiger, 

Philadelphia,  Pennsylvania. 
Hartroft,  P.  M.,  and  Eisenstein,  A.  B.  (1957).  Alterations  in  the  adrenal  cortex  of  the 

rat  induced  by  sodium  deficiency:  Correlation  of  histologic  changes  with  steroid 

hormone  secretion.  Endocrinology  60,  641-651. 
Hatfield,  D.  M.  (1935).  A  natural  history  study  of  Microtus  calif ornicus.  J.  Mammal. 

16,  261-270. 
Hellman,  K.,  and  Collins,  J.  (1957).  Thyroid,  salivary  and  Harderian  glands  in  mice 

exposed  to  heat.  J.  Endocrinol.  15,  145-150. 
Hess,  M.  (1953).  Effect  of  thyroidectomy  in  adrenal  weight  in  adult  male  rats.  Proc. 

Soc.  Exptl.  Biol.  Med.  84,  127-130. 
Hess,  M.,  and  Finerty,  J.  C.  (1952).  The  effect  of  hypo-  and  hyper-thyroidism  on  the 

pituitary-adrenal  response  to  stress.  Texas  Repts.  Biol,  and  Med.  10,  554-570. 
Hess,  M.,  Hall,  O.,  Hall,  C.  E.,  and  Finerty,  J.  C.  (1952).  Endocrine  factors  effecting  the 

weight  and  ascorbic  acid  content  of  rat  preputial  glands.  Proc.  Soc.  Exptl.  Biol.  Med. 

79,  290-292. 
Hess,  M.,  Rennels,  E.  G.,  and  Finerty,  J.  C.  (1953).  Response  of  preputial  and  adrenal 

glands  of  hypophysectomized  rats  to  ACTH.  Endocrinology  52,  223-227. 
Hill,  R.  T.  (1937).  Ovaries  secrete  male  hormone.  I.  Restoration  of  the  castrate  tj-pe  of 

seminal  vesicle  and  prostate  glands  to  normal  by  grafts  of  ovaries  on  mice.  Endo- 
crinology 21,  495-502. 


342  J.  J.   Christian 

Hill,  R.  T.   (1948).  Grafted  mouse  ovaries  and  their  adrenal  cortical  function.  Endo- 

crinoloyy  42,  339-351. 
Hoffmann,  R.  S.  (195S).  The  role  of  reproduction  and  mortality  in  population  fluctuations 

of  voles  [Alicrotus).  Ecol.  Monoc/raphs  28,  79-109. 
Hofmann,  F.  CJ.  (19.)6).  Olweivations  on  in  vitro  adrenal  steroid  synthesis  in  the  albino 

mouse.  Endocrinology  59,  712-715. 
Hofmann,  F.  G.  (1957).  The  in  vitro  hydroxjdation  of  21-carbon  steroids  by  rat  adrenal 

glands.  Endocrinology  60,  382-389. 
Holmes,  W.  N.  (1955).  Histological  variations  in  the  adrenal  cortex  of  the  golden  hamster 

with  special  reference  to  the  X-zone.  Anat.  Record  122,  271-294. 
Horn,  E.  H.  (1955).  Nutritional  and  hormonal  influences  upon  reproductive  maturation, 

organ  weights  and  histochemistr}-  of  immature  male  rat.  Endocrinology  57,  399-40S. 
Howard,  E.  (1928).  A  transitory  zone  in  the  adrenal  cortex  which  shows  age  and  sex 

relationships.  Am.  J.  Anat.  40,  251-293. 
H(jward,  E.  (1938).  The  representation  of  the  adrenal  X-zone  in  rats,  in  the  light  of 

observations  on  X-zone  variability  in  mice.  Am.  J.  Anat.  62,  351-373. 
Howard,  E.  (1939).  A  note  on  the  reactions  of  the  mouse  uterus  to  ovariectomy  in  tlu' 

presence  of  the  X-zone.  Bull.  Johns  Hopkins  Hosp.  65,  341-347. 
Howard,  E.  (1946).  The  effect  of  adrenalectomy  on  the  accessory  reproductive  glands  of 

mice  castrated  for  short  periods.  Endocrinology  38,  156-164. 
Howard,  E.  (1959).  A  complementary  action  of  corticosterone  and  deh^^droepiuandro- 

sterone  on  the  mouse  adrenal  with  observations  on  the  reactivity  of  reproductive 

tract    structures    to    dehydroepiandrosterone    and     11-hydroxy-androstenedione. 

Endocrinology  65,  785  801. 
Howard,  E.,  and  Benua,  R.  S.  (1950).  The  effect  of  protein  deficiency  and  other  dietary- 
factors  on  the  X-zone  of  the  mouse  adrenal.  /.  Nutrition  42,  157-174. 
Hsieh,  A.  C.  L.,  and  Carlson,  L.  D.  (1957a).  Role  of  adrenaline  and  noradrenaline  in 

chemical  regulation  of  heat  production.  Am.  J.  Physiol.  190,  243-246. 
Hsieh,  A.  C.  L.,  Carlson,  L.  D.,  and  Gray,  G.  (1957b).  Role  of  sympathetic  nervous 

system  in  the  control  of  chemical  regulation  of  heat  production.  A))i.  J.  Physiol.  190, 

247-251. 
Huggins,  C,  Parsons,  F.  M.,  and  Jensen,  E.  V.  (1955).  Promotion  of  growth  of  preputial 

glands  b}^  steroids  and  the  pituitary  growth  hormone.  Endocrinology  57,  25-32. 
Hughes,  E.,  and  Mall,  R.  (1958).  Relation  of  the  adrenal  cortex  to  condition  of  deer. 

Calif.  Fish  Game  44,  191-195. 
Hume,  D.  M.  (1953).  The  neuro-endocrine  response  to  injury-,  present  status  of  the 

problem.  Ann.  Surg.  138,  548-557. 
Hurst,  v.,  and  Turner,  C.  W.  (1947).  The  thyroid  secretion  rate  of  growing  and  mature 

mice.  Am.  J.  Physiol.  150,  686-692. 
Ingalls,  T.  H.  (1956).  Causes  and  prevention  of  developmental  defects.  J.  Am.  Med. 

Assoc.  161,  1048-1051. 
Ingalls,  T.  H.,  and  Philbrook,  F.  R.  (1958).  Monstrosities  induced  by  hypoxia.  Netv  Engl. 

J.  Med.  259,  558-564. 
Ingle,  D.  J.  (1949).  Some  studies  on  the  role  of  the  adrenal  cortex  in  organic  metabolism. 

Ann.  N.  Y.  Acad.  Sci.  50,  576-595. 
Ingle,  D.  J.  (1950).  The  biologic  proj>erties  of  cortisone:  A  review   J.  Clin.  Endocrinol. 

10,  1312-1354. 
Irving,  J.  T.  (1957).  A  compar  son  of  the  influence  of  hormones,  vitamins,  and  other 

dietary  factors  upon  the  formation  of  bone,  dentine,  and  enamel.   Vitamins  and 

Hormones  15,  291-232. 


2.   Endocrities  and  Populations  343 

Isler,  H.,  Leblond,  C.  P.,  and  Axelrod,  A.  A.  (1958).  Mechanism  of  the  thyrod  stimu- 
lation produced  by  sodium  chloride  in  the  mouse.  Endocrinology  62,  159-172. 
Jacot,  B.,  and  Sel.ye,  H.  (1951).  A  non-adrenal  mediated  action  of  ACTH  on  the  preputial 

glands.  Proc.  Soc.  Exptl.  Biol.  Med.  78,  46-48. 
Jameson,  E.  W.,  Jr.  (1953).  Reproduction  of  deer  mice  (Peromyscus  maniculatus  and  P. 

hoylii)  in  the  Sierra  Nevada,  California.  J.  Mammal.  34,  44-58. 
Jameson,  E.  W.,  Jr.  (1955).  Some  factors  affecting  fluctuations  of  Microlus  and  Pero- 

mycus.  J.  Mammal.  36,  206-209. 
Jones,  G.  E.  S.,  Delfa,  E.,  and  Foot,  E.  C.  (1946).  The  effect  of  thiouracil  hypothyroidism 

on  reproduction  in  the  rat.  Endocrinology  38,  337-344. 
Jones,  I.  C.  (1948).  Relationship  of  the  X  zone  of  the  mouse  adrenal.  Proc.  Soc.  Exptl. 

Biol.  Med.  69,  120-121. 
Jones,  I.  C.  (1949a).  The  action  of  testosterone  on  the  adrenal  corte.x  of  hypophysecto- 

mized  prepuberally  castrated  mouse.  Endocrinology  44,  427-438. 
Jones,  I.  C.   (1949b).  The  relationship  of  the  mouse  adrenal  cortex  to  the  ])ituitaiy. 

Endocrinology  45,  514-536. 
Jones,  I.  C.  (1950).  The  effect  of  hj^pophysectomy  on  the  adrenal  cortex  of  the  immature 

mouse.  Am.  J.  Anat.  86,  371-403. 
Jones,  I.  C.  (1952).  The  disappearance  of  the  X  zone  of  the  mouse  adrenal  cortex  during 

first  pregnancy.  Proc.  Roy.  Soc.  B139,  398-410. 
Jones,  I.  C.  (1957).  "The  Adrenal  Cortex."  Cambridge  Univ.  Press,  London  and  New 

York. 
Jones,  I.  C,  and  Rob}-,  C.  C.   (1954).  Some  aspects  of  zonation  and  function  of  the 

adrenal  cortex.  I.  The  effects  of  hypophysectomy  on  the  adrenal  cortex  of  the  adult 

male  mouse.  ./.  Endocrinol.  10,  245-2.50. 
Jones,  I.  C,  and  Spalding,  M.  H.  (1954).  Some  aspects  of  zonation  and  function  of  the 

adrenal  cortex.  II.  The  rat  adrenal  after  enucleation.  J.  Endocrinol.  10,  251-261. 
Jones,  I.  C,  and  Wright,  A.  (19.54a).  Some  aspects  of  zonation  and  function  of  the 

adrenal  cortex.  III.  Self-selection  after  adrenal  enucleation  in  rats.  /.  Endocrinol 

10,  262-265. 
Jones,  I.  C,  and  Wright,  .\.   (1954b).  Some  aspects  of  zonation  and  function  of  the 

adrenal  cortex.  1\.  The  histology  of  the  adrenal  in  rats  with  diabetes  insipidus. 

./.  Endocrinol.  10,  266-272. 
Josimovich,  J.  B.,  Ladman,  .\.  J.,  and  Deane,  H.  W.  (1954).  A  histophysiological  study 

of  the  developing  adrenal  cortex  of  the  rat  during  fetal  and  earl.\'  post  natal  stages. 

Endocrinology  54,  627-639. 
Kalela,  O.  (1957).  Regulation  of  reproduction  rate  in  subarctic  populations  of  the  vole 

Clethrionomys  rufocamis  {Sund.).  Ann.  Sci.  Fennicae,  Ser.  A.  Iv.  Biol.  34,  1-60. 
Kalter,  H.  (1954).  The  inheritance  of  susceptibility  to  the  teratogenic  action  of  cortisone 

in  mice.  Genetics  39,  185-196. 
Kalter,  H.  (1956).  Modification  of  teratogenic  action  of  cortisone  in  mice  h\  maternal 

age,  maternal  weight  and  litter  size.  Am.  J.  Physiol.  185,  65-68. 
Kass,  E.  H.,  Kendrick,  M.  I.,  and  Finland,  M.  (1953a).  Effect  of  certain  corticosteroids 

and  of  growth  hormone  on  nucleoproteiiis  of  Ivmph  nodes.  Ann.  .V.  Y.  Acad.  Sci. 

56,  737-741. 
Kass,  E.  H.,  Lundgren,  M.  M.,  and  Finland,  M.  (1953b).  Observations  on  the  effect  of 

corticosteroids  and  growth  hormone  on  resistance  to  experimental  pneumococcal 

and  influenza  virus  infections.  Ann.  N.  Y.  Acad.  Sci.  56,  76.5-771. 
Kass,  E.  H.,  Kendrick,  M.  I.,  and  Finland,  M.  (1955).  Effects  of  corticosterone,  hydro- 
cortisone and  corticotroi)in  on  production  of  antibodies  in  ral)bits.  J.  Exptl.   Med. 

102,  767-774. 


344  ,/.  J.   Christian 

Kelsall,  M.  A.,  and  Crabb,  E.  D.  (1958).  Lymphocytes  and  plasmacj'tes  in  nucleoprotein 

metabolism.  Ann.  N.  Y.  Acad.  Sci.  72,  293-338. 
Keuning,  F.  J.,  and  Van  der  Slikke,  L.  B.  (1950).  The  role  of  immature  plasma  cells, 

lymphoblasts,  and  lymphocytes  in  the  formation  of  antibodies,  as  established  in 

tissue  culture  experiments.  /.  Lab.  Clin.  Med.  36,  167-182. 
Kligman,  A.  M.,  Baldridge,  G.  D.,  Rebell,  G.,  and  Pillsbury,  D.  M.  (1951).  The  effect  of 

cortisone  on  pathologic  responses  of  guinea  pigs  infected  cutaneously  with  fungi, 

viruses,  and  bacteria.  J.  Lab.  Clin.  Med.  37,  615-620. 
Knigge,  K.  M.   (1954a).  The  effect  of  hypophysectomy  on  the  adrenal  gland  of  the 

hamster  {Mesocricetus  auratus).  Am.  J.  Anat.  94,  225-271. 
Knigge,  K.  M.  (1954b).  The  effect  of  acute  starvation  of  the  adrenal  cortex  of  the 

hamster.  Anat.  Record  120,  555-582. 
Knisely,  M.  H.,  Bloch,  E.  H.,  Ehot,  T.  S.,  and  Warner,  L.  (1947).  Sludged  blood.  Science 

106,  431-440. 
Kraatz,  C.  P.  (1939).  Effect  of  brief  experimental  hyperthyroidism  on  reproduction  in 

the  rat.  Proc.  Soc.  Exptl.  Biol.  Med.  40,  499-502. 
Kracht,  J.  (1954).  Fright-thyrotoxicosis  in  the  wild  rabljit,  a  model  of  thyrotrophic 

alarm-reaction.  Acta  Endocrinol.  15,  355-367. 
Kramer,  J.,  Meyers,  V.  M.,  McCarthy,  H.  H.,  and  Simay  Kramer,  M.  (1957).  Further 

study  on  the  endocrine  relations  of  capillary  resistance.  Endocrinology  60,  589-596. 
Kuhl,  W.  J.,  Jr.,  and  Ziff,  M.  (1952).  Alteration  of  thyroid  function  by  ACTH  and 

cortisone.  /.  Clin.  Endocrinol,  and  Metabolism  12,  555-559. 
Lanman,  J.  T.  (1957).  The  adrenal  fetal  zone:  Its  occurrence  in  primates  and  a  possible 

relationship  to  chorionic  gonadotropin.  Endocrinology  61,  684-691. 
Laqueur,  G.  L.,  McCann,  S.  M.,  Schreiner,  L.  H.,  Rosemberg,  E.,  Rioch,  D.,  and  Ander- 
son, E.  (1955).  Alterations  of  adrenal  cortical  and  ovarian  activity  following  hypo- 
thalamic lesions.  Endocrinology  57,  44-54. 
Leathern,  J.  H.  (1950).  Seminal  vesicle  response  as  an  assessment  of  androgenic  activity. 

Trans.  N.  Y.  Acad.  Sci.  [2],  12,  234-235. 
LeMaistre,  C.  A.,  Tompsett,  R.,  and  McDermott,  W.  (1953).  The  effects  of  cortico- 
steroids upon  tuberculosis  and  pseudotuberculosis.  Ann.  N.    Y.  Acad.  Sci.   56, 

772-778. 
Levy,  A.  C,  and  Ramey,  E.  R.  (1958).  Effect  of  autonomic  blocking  agents  on  depot 

fat  mobilization  in  normal  adrenalectomized  animals.  Proc.  Soc.  Exptl.  Biol.  Med. 

99,  637-639. 
Li,  C.  H.,  F0nss-Bech,  P.,  Geschwind,  I.  I.,  Hayashida,  T.,  Hungerford,  G.  F.,  Lostroh, 

A.  J.,  Lyons,  W.  R.,  Moon,  H.  D.,  Reinhardt,  W.  O.,  and  Sideman,  M.  B.  (1957). 

Corticotropins  (ACTH)  X.  Biological  investigations  of  a-corticotropin.  /.  Exptl. 

Med.  105,  335-360. 
Liddle,  G.  W.,  Duncan,  L.  E.,  and  Bartter,  F.  C.  (1956).  Dual  mechanism  regulating 

adrenocortical  function  in  man.  Metabolism  5,  369-383. 
Lillie,  R.  D.  (1954).  Histopathologic  Technic  and  Practical  Histochemistry.  McGraw- 
Hill  (Blakiston),  New  York. 
Lord,  R.  A.  (1958).  Studies  on  the  use  of  cortisone  and  ACTH  in  trichinosis.  Am.  J. 

Trop.  Med.  Hyg.  7,  611-617. 
Lostroh,  A.  J.,  and  Li,  C.  H.  (1957).  Stimulation  of  the  sex  accessories  of  hypophysecto- 

mized  male  rats  by  non-gonadotrophic  hormones  of  the  pituitary  gland.  Acta 

Endocrinol.  25,  1-16. 
Lostroh,  A.  J.,  and  Li,  C.  H.  (1958).  Effect  of  growth  hormone  and  thyroxine  on  body 

weight  of  hypophysectomized  C3H  mice.  Endocrinology  62,  484-492. 


2.  Endocrines  and  Populations  345 

Lostroh,  A.  J.,  and  Woodward,  P.  (1958).  Changes  in  the  adrenal  of  hypophysectomizc^d 
C3H  mouse  with  a-corticotropin  and  growth  hormone.  Endocrinology  62,  498-505. 
Louch,  C,  Meyer,  R.  K.,  and  Emlen,  J.  T.  (1953).  Effect  of  stress  on  diurnal  fluctuations 

in  eosinophils  of  the  laboratory  mouse  Proc.  Soc.  Exptl.  Biol.  Med.  82,  G68-671. 
Louch,  C.  D.  (1956).  Adrenocortical  activity  in  relation  to  the  density  and  dynamics  of 

three  confined  populations  of  Microlus  pennsylvanicus.  Ecology  37,  701-713. 
Louch,   C.  D.    (1958).  Adrenocortical  activity  in  two  meadow  vole  populations.  J. 

Mammal.  39,  109-116. 
Lutwak-Mann,  C.  (1958).  The  dependence  of  gonadal  function  upon  vitamins  and  other 

nutritional  factors.  Vitamins  and  Hormones  16,  35-75. 
Lutwak-Mann,  C,  Mann,  T.,  and  Price,  D.  (1949).  Metabolic  activity  in  tissue  trans- 
plants. Hormone  induced  formation  of  fructose  and  citric  acid  in  transplants  from 

accessory  glands  of  reproduction.  Proc.  Roy.  Soc.  B136,  461-471, 
McCann,  S.  M.  (1953).  Effect  of  hypothalamic  lesions  on  the  adrenal  cortical  response 

to  stress  in  the  rat.  Am.  J.  Physiol.  175,  13-20. 
McCann,  S.  M.  (1957).  The  ACTH-releasing  activity  of  extracts  of  the  posterior  lobe  of 

the  pituitary  in  vivo.  Endocrinology  60,  664-676. 
McCann,  S.  M.,  and  Brobeck,  J.  R.  (1954).  Evidence  for  a  role  of  the  supraopticohypo- 

physeal  system  in  regulation  of  adrenocorticotrophin  secretion.  Proc.  Soc.  Exptl. 

Biol.  Med.  87,  318-324. 
McCann,  S.  M.,  and  Fruit,  A.  (1957).  Eflfect  of  synthetic  vasopressin  on  release  of 

adrenocorticotrophin  in  rats  with  hypothalamic  lesions.  Proc.  Soc.  Exptl.  Biol.  Med. 

96,  566-567. 
McDonald,  R.  K.,  Wagner,  H.  N.,  Jr.,  and  Wiese,  V.  K.  (1957).  Relationship  between 

endogenous  antidiuretic  hormone  activity  and  ACTH  release  in  man.  Proc.  Soc. 

Exptl.  Biol.  Med.  96,  652-655. 
MacLulich,  D.  A.  (1937).  Fluctuations  in  the  numbers  of  the  varying  hare.  Univ.  Toronto 

Studies  Biol.  Ser.  43,  1-136. 
McPhail,  M.  K.  (1944).  Androgenic  activity  of  the  adrenal  gland  with  special  reference  to 

the  X-zone.  Rev.  can.  hiol.  3,  312-327. 
McPhail,  M.  K.,  and  Read,  H.  C.  (1942a).  The  mouse  adrenal.  I.  Development,  de- 
generation, and  regeneration  of  the  X-zone.  Anat.  Record  84,  51-73. 
McPhail,  M.  K.,  and  Read,  H.  C.  (1942b).  The  mouse  adrenal.  II.  The  action  of  certain 

hormonal  substances  on  the  adrenal  gland  of  the  mouse  with  particular  reference  to 

their  action  on  the  X-zone.  Anat.  Record  84,  75-89. 
Mann,  T.  (1954).  On  the  presence  and  role  of  inositol  and  certain  other  substances  in  the 

seminal  vesicle  secretion  of  the  boar.  Proc.  Roy.  Soc.  B142,  21-32. 
Maqsood,  M.,  and  Reineke,  E.  P.  (1950).  Conference  of  environmental  temperatures  and 

thyroid  status  on  sexual  development  in  male  mouse.  Am.  J.  Physiol.  162,  24-30. 
Marine,  D.,  and  Bauman,  E.  J.  (1945).  Hypertrophy  of  adrenal  medulla  of  white  rats  in 

chronic  thiouracil  poisoning.  Am.  J.  Physiol.  144,  69-73. 
Markee,  J.  E.,  Everett,  J.  W.,  and  Sawyer,  C.  H.  (1952),  The  relationship  of  the  nervous 

system  to  the  release  of  gonadotrophin  and  the  regulation  of  the  sex  cycle.  Recent 

Progr.  in  Hormone  Research  7,  139-163. 
Marrian,  G.  F.,  and  Parkes,  A.  S.  (1929).  The  effect  of  anterior  pituitary  preparations 

administered  during  dietary  anestrus.  Proc.  Roy.  Soc.  B105,  248-258. 
Martin,  E.  P.  (1956).  A  population  study  of  the  prairie  vole  {Microtus  ochrogasler)  in 

Northeastern  Kansas.  Univ.  Kansas  Museum  Nat.  Hist.  Publ.  8,  361-416. 
Martorano,  J.  J.  (1957).  Variation  in  liver  glycogen  levels  of  intact  and  adrenalectomized 

mice.  Naval  Med.  Res.  Inst.  Res  Rept.  15,  191-198. 


346  ./.  J.  Christian 

Mason,  J.  W.   (1959).  Central  nervous  system  factors  in  the  regulation  of  endocrine 

secretion.  Recent  Progr.  in  Hormone  Research,  15,  345-3(S9. 
Meier,  R.,  Schuler,  W.,  and  DesauUes,  P.   (1950).  Zur  Frage  des  Mechanismus  der 

Hemmung  des  Bindegewebswachstume  durch  Cortisone.  Experientia  6,  469-471. 
Menkin,  V.  (1955).  Factors  concerned  in  the  mobilization  of  leukocytes  in  inflammation. 

Ann.  N.  Y.  Acad.  Sci.  59,  956-958. 
Miller,  R.  A.  (1949).  Cytological  criteria  of  activity  and  inactivity  in  the  glomerular 

fascicular  zones  of  the  adrenal  cortex  in  mice.  Anat  Record  103,  489. 
Mills,  I.  H.,  Casper,  A.,  and  Bartter,  F.  C.  (1958).  On  the  role  of  the  vagus  in  the  control 

of  aldosterone  secretion.  Science  128,  1140-1141. 
Money,  W.  L.  (1955).  The  interrelation  of  the  thyroid  and  the  adrenals.  Brookhaven 

Symposia  in  Biol.  No.  7,  137-168. 
Money,  W.  L.,  Kirschner,  L.,  Kraintz,  L.,  Merrill,  P.,  and  Rawson,  R.  W.  (1950).  Effects 

of  adrenal  and  gonadal  products  on  the  weight  and  radioiodine  uptake  of  the 

thyroid  gland  in  the  rat.  J .  Clin.  Endocrinol.  10    1282-1295. 
Moon,  H.  D.,  Simpson,  M.  E.,  Li,  C.  H.,  and  Evans,  H.  M.  (1950).  Neoplasms  in  rats 

treated  with  pituitary  growth  hormone.  II.   Adrenal  gland.  Cancer  Research  10, 

364-370. 
Moon,  H.  D.,  Simpson,  M.  E.,  Li,  C.  H.,  and  Evans,  H.  M.  (1951).  Neoplasms  in  rats 

treated  with  pituitary  growth  hormone.  V.  Absence  of  neoplasms  in  hypophysecto- 

mized  rats.  Cancer  Research  11,  535-539. 
Morin,  G.  (1946a).  MeduUo-surrenale  et  regulation  thermique.  I.  Action  calorigene  de 

I'adrenaline.  Demonstration,  signification.  Rev.  can.  hiol.  5,  121-134. 
Morin,  G.  (1946b).  MeduUo-surrenale  et  regulation  thermique.  II.  Action  adrenalino- 

secr^trice  du  froid.  Rev.  can.  hiol.  5,  388-400. 
Mosier,  H.  D.  (1957).  Comparative  histological  study  of  the  adrenal  cortex  of  the  wild 

and  domesticated  Norway  rat.  Endocrinology  60,  460-469. 
Moss,  M.  L.  (1955).  Morphological  changes  in  the  growing  rat  skull  following  administra- 
tion of  cortisone  acetate.  Proc.  Soc.  Expll.  Biol.  Med.  89,  648-650. 
Mulinos,  M.  G.,  and  Pomerantz,  L.  (1941).  Hormonal  influences  on  the  weight  of  the 

adrenal  in  inanition.  Am.  J .  Physiol.  132,  368-374. 
Mulinos,  M.  G.,  Pomerantz,  L.,  Smelser,  J.,  and  Kurzrok,  R.  (1939).  Estrus-inhibiting 

effects  of  inanition.  Proc.  Soc.  Exptl.  Biol.  Med.  40,  79-83. 
Nalbandov,  A.  V.,  Moore,  W.  W.,  and  Norton,  H.  W.  (1955).  Further  studies  on  the 

neurogenic  control  of  the  estrus  cycle  liy  uterine  distension.   Endocrinology  56, 

225-231. 
Nelson,  D.  H.  (1955).  The  significance  of  plasma  corticosteroids  as  a  measure  of  adrenal 

cortical  function.  In  "Symposium  on  Adrenal  Functions  in  Infants  and  Children," 

N.  Y.  Univ.  Med.  Ctr.,  Syracuse,  1954.  NMRI  (Naval  Med.  Res.  Inst.)  L  and  R 

55-1. 
Nichols,  J.  (19.50).  Effects  of  captivity  on  adrenal  gland  of  wild  Norway  rat.  Am.  J. 

Physiol.  162,  5-9. 
Noble,  R.  L.  (1955).  Physiology  of  the  adrenal  cortex.  In  "The  Hormones"  (G.  Pincus 

and  K.  V.  Thimann,  eds.),  \o\.  Ill,  pp.  685-819.  Academic  Press,  New  York. 
Nowell,  N.  W.  (1959).  Studies  in  the  activation  and  inhibition  of  adrenocorticotrophin 

secretion.  Endocrinology  64,  191-201. 
Odum,  E.  P.  (1955).  An  eleven  year  history  of  a  Sigmodon  population.  ./.  Man^iud.  36, 

368-378. 
Parmer,  L.  G.,  Katonah,  F.,  and  Angrist,  A.  A.  (1951).  Comparative  effects  of  ACTH, 

cortisone,  corticosterone,  desoxycorticosterone,  pregnenolone  on  growth  and  de- 
velopment of  infant  rats.  Proc.  Soc.  E.rptl.  Biol.  Med.  77,  21.5-218. 


2.  Endocrines  and  Populations  347 

Paschkis,  K.  E.,  Cantarow,  A.,  Eberhard,  T.,  and  Doyle,  D.  (1950).  Thyroid  funftion  in 

the  alarm  reaction.  Froc.  Soc.  Exptl.  Biol.  Med.  73,  116-118. 
Pauly,  J.  E.  (1957).  Morphological  observations  on  the  adrenal  cortex  of  the  laboratory 

rat.  Endocrinology  60,  247-264. 
Peron,  F.  G.,  and  Dorfman,  R.  I.  (1958).  Quantitative  analysis  of  steroids  isolated  from 

the  urine  of  normal  and  ACTH  treated  guinea  pigs.  Endocrinology  62,  1-8. 
Perry,  W.  F.  (1951).  The  action  of  cortisone  and  ACTH  on  thryoid  function.  Endo- 
crinology 49,  284-288. 
Pitelka,  F.  A.  (1957a).  Some  aspects  of  population  structure  in  the  short-term  cycle  of  the 

brown  lemming  in  Northern  Alaska.  Cold  Spring  Harbor  Symposia  Quant.  Biol. 

22,  237-251. 
Pitelka.  F.  A.   (1957b).  Some  characteristics  of  microtine  cycles  in  the  arctic.  Biol. 

Colloq.  Proc.  18th  Colloq.  73-88. 
Poindexter,  H.  A.  (1949).  Experimental  animal  colony  in  tropical  West  Africa.  Public 

Health  Repts.  {U.  S.)  64,  57-62. 
Pollard.  M.,  and  Wilson,  B.  R.  (1955).  The  prolongation  of  murine  typus  rickettsemia  by 

cortisone.  Texas  Repts.  Biol,  and  Med.  13,  126-128. 
Poore,  W.,  and  Hollander,  V.  P.  (1957).  Plasma  ketosteriod  studies  in  normal  mice. 

Endocrinology  61,  651-656. 
Porter,  J.  C,  and  Jones,  I.  C.  (1956).  Effect  of  plasma  from  hypophyseal-portal  vessel 

blood  on  adrenal  ascorbic  adid.  Endocrinology  58,  62-67. 
Porter,  J.  C,  and  Rumsfeld,  H.  W.,  Jr.  (1956).  Effects  of  lyophilized  plasma  and  plasma 

fractions  from  h3-pophvseal-portal  vessel  blood  on  adrenal  ascorbic  acid.  Endo- 
crinology 58,  359-364. 
Preble,  E.  A.   (1908).  A  biological  investigation  of  the  Athabaska-Mackenzie  region. 

U.  S.  Dept.  Agr.  N.  Am.  Fauna  No.  27. 
Puntriano,  G.,  and  Meites,  J.   (1951).  The  effects  of  continuous  light  or  darkness  on 

th^-roid    function    in    mice.    Endocrinology    48,    217-224. 
Ramey,  E.  R.,  and  Goldstein,  M.  S.  (1957).  The  adrenal  cortex  and  the  sympathetic 

nervous  system.  Physiol.  Revs.  37,  155-195. 
Randall,  H.  T.  (1952).  The  shifts  of  fluid  and  electrolytes  in  shock.  Ann.  N.  Y.Acad. 

Sci.  55,  412-428. 
Ratcliffe,  H.  L.,  and  Cronin,  M.  T.  I.  (1958).  Changing  frequency  of  arteriosclerosis  in 

mammals  and  birds  at  the  Philadelphia  Zoological  Gardens.  Circtilation  18,  41-52. 
Rausch,  R.  (1950).  Observations  on  a  cyclic  decline  of  lemmings  {Lemmus)  on  the  arctic 

coast  of  Alaska  during  the  spring  of  1949.  Arctic  3,  166-177. 
Rawson,  R.  W.,  Rail,  J.  E.,  and  Sonenberg,  M.  (1955).  The  chemistry  and  physiology  of 

the  thyroid.  In  "The  Hormones"  (G.  Pincus  and  K.  V.  Thimann,  eds),  Vol.  Ill, 

pp.  433-519.  Academic  Press,  New  York. 
Reichlin,  S.  (1957a).  The  effect  of  dehydration,  starvation,  and  pitressin  injections  on 

thyroid  activity  in  the  rat.  Endocrinology  60,  470-487. 
Reichlin,  S.  (1957b).  The  effect  of  hypothalamic  lesions  upon  the  thyroid  response  to 

partial  thyroidectomy.  Endocrinology  60,  567-569. 
Reif,  A.  E.,  and  Longwell,  B.  B.  (1958).  Extraction  of  corticosteroids  from  adrenal  vein 

blood  of  rats  and  rabbits.  Endocrinology  62,  573-586. 
Renuels,  E.  G.,  and  Drager,  G.  A.  (1955).  The  relationship  of  pituicytes  to  neurosecretion. 

Anat.  Record  122,  193-203. 
Rennels,  E.  G.,  Hess,  M.,  and  Finerty,  J.  C.  (1953).  Response  of  preputial  and  adrenal 

glands  of  the  rat  to  sex  hormones.  Proc.  Soc.  Exptl.  Biol.  Med.  82,  304-309. 
Retzlaff,  E.  G.  (1938).  Studies  in  population  physiology  with  the  albino  mouse.  Biol. 

Generalis  14,  238-265. 


348  /.  /.  Christian 

Richards,  J.  B.,  and  Pruitt,  R.  L.  (1957).  Hydrocortisone  suppression  of  stress-induced 

adrenal  17-hydroxycorticosteroid  secretion  in  dogs.  Endocrinology  60,  99-104. 
Richardson,  J.  A.,  and  Woods,  E.  F.  (19.59).  Release  of  norepinephrine  from  the  isolated 

heart.  Proc.  Soc.  Exptl.  Biol.  Med.  100,  149-151. 
Richter,  C.  P.,  Rogers,  P.  V.,  and  Hall,  C.  E.  (1950).  Failure  of  salt  replacement  therapy 

in  adrenalectomized  recently  captured  wild  Norway  rats.  Endocrinology  46,  233-242. 
Ring,  G.  C.  (1942).  The  importance  of  the  thyroid  in  maintaining  an  adequate  production 

of  heat  during  exposure  to  cold.  Am.  J.  Physiol.  137,  582-588. 
Robert,  A.,  and  Nezamis,  J.  E.  (1957).  The  granuloma  pouch  as  a  routine  assay  for 

antiphlogistic  compounds.  Acta  Endocrinol.  25,  105-112. 
Robinson,  H.  J.,  and  Smith,  A.  L.  (1953).  The  effect  of  adrenal  cortical  hormones  on 

experimental  infection.  Ann.  N.  Y.  Acad.  Sci.  56,  757-764. 
Rogers,  P.  V.,  and  Richter,  C.  P.  (1948).  Anatomical  comparison  between  the  adrenal 

glands  of  wild  Norwaj-,  wild  alexandrine  and  domestic  Norway  rats.  Endocrinology 

42,  46-55. 
Rosemberg,  E.,  Cornfield,  J.,  Bates,  R.  W.,  Anderson,  E.  (1954).  Bioassay  of  adrenal 

steroids  in  blood  and  urine  based  on  eosinophil  response:  a  statistical  analj'sis. 

Endocronilogy  54,  363-383. 
Rosenfeld,  G.  (1958).  The  function  and  capacity  of  the  adrenal  cortex  immediately 

before  radiation  sickness  death.  J.  Lab.  Clin.  Med.  51,  198-210. 
Rosenfeld,  G.,  and  Bascom,  W.  D.  (1956).  In  vitro  studies  of  the  influence  of  corticotropin, 

somatotropin,   thj-rotropin  and  gonadotropin  on  adrenal  steroidogenesis.   Endo- 
crinology 59,  497-499. 
Rothballer,  A.  B.  (1953).  Changes  in  the  rat  neurohypophysis  of  painful  stimuli  with 

particular  reference  to  neurosecretory  material.  Anat.  Record  115,  21-41. 
Saffran,  M.,  Schally,  A.  V.,  and  Benfey,  B.  G.  (1955).  Stimulation  of  the  release  of 

corticotrophin  from  the  adenoh3'pophysis  by  a  neurohypophj'seal  factor.  Endo- 
crinology 57,  439-444. 
Sakiz,  E.  (1956).  Le  cortex  surrenal  exerce-t-il  une  action  hormonale  sexuelle?  Corn-pi. 

rend.  soc.  biol.  150,  2172-2174. 
Salter,  W.  T.  (1949).  The  metabolic  circuit  of  thyroid  hormone.  Ann.  N.  Y.  Acad.  Sci.  50, 

358-376. 
Sandberg,  A.  A.,  Nelson,  D.  M.,  Palmer,  J.  G.,  Samuel,  L.  T.,  and  Tyler,  F.  H.  (1953). 

The  effects  of  epinephrine  on  the  metabolism  of  17-h3'dro.xycorticosteroids  in  the 

human.  /.  Clin.  Endocrinol,  and  Metabolism  13,  629-647. 
Santisteban,  G.  A.,  and  Doughert}-,  T.  F.  (1954).  Comparison  of  the  influence  of  adreno- 
cortical hormones  on  the  growth  and  involution  of  lymphatic  organs.  Endocrinology 

54,  130-146. 
Sayers,  G.,  and  Sayers,  M.  (1949).  The  pituitary-adrenal  system.  Ann.  N.  Y.  Acad.  Sci. 

50,  522-539. 
Schally,  A.  V.,  and  Guillemin,  R.  (1959).  Concentration  of  corticotropin  releasing  factor 

by  chromatography  on  carboxymethylcellulose.  Proc.  Soc.  Exptl.  Biol.  Med.  100, 

138-139. 
Schapiro,  S.,  Marmorston,  J.,  and  Sobel,  H.  (1958).  Mobilization  of  the  antidiuretic 

hormone  and  the  secretion  of  ACTH  following  cold  stress.  Endocrinology  62,  278-282. 
Scharrer,  E.,  and  Scharrer,  B.    (1954).   Hormones  produced  by  neurosecretorj--  cells. 

Recent  Progr.  in  Hormone  Research  10,  183-240. 
Schindler,  W.  J.,  and  Knigge,  K.  M.  (1959a).  Adrenal  cortical  secretion  by  the  golden 

hamster.  Endocrinology  65,  739-747. 
Schindler.  W.  J.,  and  Knigge,  K.  M.  (1959b).  In  vitro  studies  and  adrenal  steroidogenesis 

by  the  golden  hamster.  Endocrinology  65,  748-765. 


2.  Endocrines  and  Populations  349 

Schmidt,  L.  H.,  and  Squires,  W.  L.  (1951j.  The  influence  of  cortisone  on  primate  malaria. 

/.  Exptl.  Med.  94,  501-520. 
Schmiterlow,  C.  G.  (1948).  The  nature  and  occurrence  of  pressor  and  depressor  sub- 
stances in  extracts  from  blood  vessels.  Acta  Physiol.  Scand.  16,  Siippl.  56,  1-113. 
Scudder,  J.  (1952).  Introduction  to  shock  syndrome.  Ann.  N.  Y.  Acad.  Sci.  55,  3-49-350. 
Sealander,  J.  A.,  Jr.  (1950).  Effect  of  environmental  temperature  and  starvation  on 

adrenal  glands  of  the  white-footed  mouse.  Am.  J.  Physiol.  163,  92-95. 
Seifter,  J.,  Ehrich,  W.  E.,  and  Hud^-ma,  G.  M.  (1949).  Effect  of  prolonged  administration 

of  antithyroid  compounds  on  the  th^-roid  and  other  endocrine  organs  of  the  rat. 

A.  M.  A.  Arch.  Pathol.  48,  536-547. 
Seifter,  J.,  Christian,  J.  J.,  and  Ehrich,  W.  E.  (1951).  The  effect  of  cortisone  and  other 

steroids  on  the  hibernating  gland  of  the  pregnant  white  rat.  Federation  Proc.  10, 

334. 
Seifter,  J.,  Ehrich,  W.  E.,  Baeder,  D.  H.,  Butt,  A.  J.,  and  Hauser,  E.  A.  (1953).  Evidence 

for  the  direct  effect  of  steroids  on  the  ground  substance.  Ann.  N.  Y .  Acad.  Sci.  55, 

696-697. 
Seifter,  J.,  Baeder,  D.  H.,  Zarafonetis,  C.  J.  D,,  and  Kalas,  J.  (1959).  Effect  of  adrenals, 

pituitar}^  hver,   and  mucopolysaccharides  on  blood  lipids.   In   "Hormones  and 

Atherosclerosis"  (G.  Pincus,  ed.),  pp.  265-2S1.  Academic  Press,  New  York. 
Selye,  H.  (1939).  The  effect  of  adaptation  to  various  damaging  agents  on  the  female  sex 

organs  in  the  rat.  Endocrinology  25,  61.5-624. 
Selye,  H.  (1947).  "Textbook  of  Endocrinology."  Acta,  Montreal. 
Selye,  H.  (1950).  "Stress."  Acta,  Montreal. 
Seh'e,  H.  (1951).  The  influence  of  STH,  ACTH  and  cortisone  upon  resistance  to  infection. 

Can.  Med.  Assoc.  J.  64,  489-494. 
Sel3-e,  H.  (1954).  Stress  and  lactation.  Rev.  can.  hiol.  13,  i,4))  377-384. 
Selye,  H.  (1955).  Interactions  between  corticoid  and  foUiculoid  hormones  in  the  regu- 
lation of  anabolism,  lymphatic  tissue  and  inflammation.  Acta  Endocrinol.  20,  1-18. 
Selye,  H.,  and  Bois,  P.  (1954).  On  the  design  of  experiments  to  appraise  the  role  of  the 

adrenals  in  biologic  reactions.  In  "4th  Annual  Report  on  Stress,"  (H.  Selye  and  G. 

Heuser,  eds.),  pp.  533-552.  Acta,  Montreal. 
Share,  L.,  and  Stadler,  J.  B.  (1958).  Alterations  in  sodium  and  potassium  metabolism 

following  hind-leg  fracture  in  the  rat:  Role  of  the  adrenal  cortex.  Endocrinology  62, 

119-128. 
Shwartzman,  G.,  and  Aronson,  S.  M.  (1953).  Pohomj^elitis  infection  by  parenteral  routes 

made  possible  bj^  cortisone.  Ann.  N .  Y .  Acad.  Sci.  56,  793-798. 
Simpson,  M.  E.,  Evans,  E.  S.,  and  Rosenberg,  L.  L.  (1959).  Re-evaluation  of  the  evidence 

for  a  pituitary  erythropoietic  hormone.  Endocrinology  64,  592-599. 
Slusher,  M.  A.  (1958).  Dissociation  of  adrenal  ascorbic  acid  and  corticosterone  response 

to  stress  in  rats  with  hypothalamic  lesions.  Endocrinology  63,  412-419. 
Smelser,  G.  K.  (1943).  Changes  induced  in  the  Harderian  gland  of  the  guinea  pig  b^-  the 

injection  of  Iwpophyseal  extracts.  Anat.  Record  86,  41-56. 
Smith,  E.  K.  (1955).  Interrelationships  of  anterior  pituitary  and  adrenal  cortex  in  the  rat 

ovarian  hyperemic  reaction.  Endocrinology  56,  567-574. 
Smith,  R.  W.,  Jr.  (1951).  Augmented  excretion  of  urine  gonadotrophins  during  ACTH 

administration.  Proc.  Soc.  Exptl.  Biol.  Med.  78,  868-872. 
Snyder,  R.  L.,  and  Christian,  J.  J.  (1960).  The  reproduction  cycle  and  the  litter  size  of 

the  southern  woodchuck.  Ecology  41,  647-656. 
Sollman,  T.  (1957).  "A  Manual  of  Pharmacology,"  8th  ed.  Saunders,  Philadelphia. 
Southern,  H.  N.  (1948).  Sexual  and  aggressive  behaviour  in  the  wild  rabbit.  Behaviour  1, 
173-194. 


350  J.  J.  Christian 

Southwick,  C.  H.  (1955a).  The  population  dynamics  of  confined  house  mice  supplied  with 

unlimited  food.  Ecologrj  36,  212-225. 
Southwick,   C.   H.    (1955b).  The  regulatory  mechanisms  of  house  mouse  population; 

social  behavior  affecting  litter  survival.  Ecology  36,  627-634. 
Southwick,  C.  H.  (1959).  Eosinophil  response  of  C57BR  mice  to  behavioral  disturbance. 

Ecology  40,  156-157. 
Speirs,  R.  S.  (1955).  Physiological  approaches  to  an  understanding  of  the  function  of 

eosinophils  and  basophils.  Ann.  N.  Y.  Acad.  Set.  59,  706-731. 
Speirs,  R.  S.,  and  Meyer,  R.  K.  (1949).  The  effects  of  stress,  adrenal  and  adrenocorti- 

cotrophic  hormones  on  the  cirulating  eosinophils  of  mice.  Endocrinology  45,  403-429. 
Speirs,  R.  S.,  and  Meyer,  R.  K.  (1951).  A  method  of  assajnng  adrenal  cortical  hormones 

based  on  a  decrease  in  the  circulating  eosinophil  cells  of  adrenalectomized  mice. 

Endocrinology  48,  316-326. 
Srebnik,  H.  H.,  Nelson,  M.  M.,  and  Simpson,  M.  E.  (1958).  Response  to  exogenous 

gonadotiophins  in  absence  of  dietary  protein.  Proc.  Soc.  Expll.  Biol.  Med.  99,  57-61. 
Stachenko,  J.,  and  Giroud,  C.  J.  P.  (1959a).  Functional  zonation  of  the  adrenal  cortex: 

Pathways  of  corticosteroid  biogenesis.  Endocrinology  64,  730-742. 
Stachenko,  J.,  and  Giroud,  C.  J.  P.  (1959b).  Functional  zonation  of  the  adrenal  cortex 

site  of  ACTH  action.  Endocrinology  64,  743-752. 
Stanbury,  S.  W.,  Gowenlock,  A.  H.,  and  Mahler,  R.  F.   (19.5S).  Interrelationships  of 

potassium  deficiency  and  renal  disease.  In  "Aldosterone,"   Intern.  Symposium, 

Geneva,  1957  (A.  F.  Muller  and  C.  M.  O'Connor,  eds.),  pp.  155-166.  Little,  Brown, 

Boston,  Massachusetts. 
Steinberger,  E.,  and  Dixon,  W.  J.  (1959).  Some  observations  on  the  effect  of  heat  on  the 

testicular  germinal  epithelium.  Fertility  and  Sterility  10,  578-595. 
Stevens,  C.  E.,  D'Angelo,  S.  A.,  Paschkis,  K.  E.,  Cantarow,  A.,  and  Sunderman,  F.  W. 

(1955).  The  response  of  the  pituitary  thyroid  system  of  the  guinea  pig  to  low  en- 
vironmental temperature.  Endocrinology  56,  143-156. 
Stoner,  R.  D.,  and  Godwin,  J.  T.   (1953).  The  effects  of  ACTH  and  cortisone  upon 

susceptibility  to  trichinosis  in  mice.  Am.  J.  Pathol.  29,  943-950. 
Story,  J.  L.,  Melby,  J.  C,  Egdahl,  R.  H.,  and  French,  L.  A.  (1959).  Adrenal  cortical 

function  following  stepwise  removal  of  the  brain  of  the  dog.  Am.  J.  Physiol.  196. 

583-588. 
Strecker,  R.  L.  (1954).  Regulatory  mechanics  in  house  mouse  populations:  The  effect  of 

limited  food  supply  on  an  unconfined  population.  Ecology  35,  249-253. 
Strecker,  R.  L.,  and  Emlen,  J.  T.  (1953).  Regulatory  mechanisms  in  house  mouse  popu- 
lations— the  effect  of  limited  food  supply  on  a  confined  population.  Ecology  34, 

375-385. 
Sturgis,  S.  H.  (1950).  The  mechanism  and  control  of  primate  ovulation.  Fertility  and 

Sterility  1,  40-52. 
Sundberg,  R.  1).  (1955).  Lymphocytes  and  plasma  cells.  Ann.  N.  Y.  Acad.  Sci.  59(5), 

671-689. 
Swanson,  H.  E.  (1956).  Interrelationships  between  thyroxin  and  adrenalin  in  the  regu- 
lation of  oxygen  consumption  on  the  albino  rat.  Endocrinology  59,  217-225. 
Swanson,  H.  E.  (1957).  The  effect  of  temperature  in  the  potentiation  of  adrenalin  by 

thyroxine  in  the  albino  rat.  Endocrinology  60,  205-213. 
Sweat,  M.  L.,  and  Farrell,  G.  L.  (1952).  Resolution  and  quantitative  analysis  of  steroids 

in  adrenal-vein  blood.  /.  Clin.  Endocrinol,  and  Metabolism  12,  168. 
Sweat,  M.  L.,  and  Farrell,  G.  L.  (1954).  Decline  of  corticosteroid  secretion  following 

hypophysectomy.  Proc.  Soc.  Exptl.  Biol.  Med.  87,  615-618. 


2.  Endocrines  and  Populations  351 

Sydnor,  K.  L.,  and  Sayers,  G.  (1954).  Blood  and  pituitary  ACTH  in  intact  and  adrenalec- 

tomized  rats  after  stress.  Endocrinology  55,  621-636. 
Sydnor,  K.  L.,  Burks,  R.,  and  Brown,  D.  (1955).  Blood  ACTH  in  the  stressed  adrenalec- 

tomized  rat  after  intravenous  injection  of  hydrocortisone.  Endocrinology  56,  204-208 
Symington,  T.,  Currie,  A.  R.,  O'Donnell,  U.  J.,  Grant,  J.  K.,  Oastler,  E.  G.,  and  Whyte, 

W.  H.  (1958).  Hyperplasia  and  tumours  of  the  human  adrenal  cortex:  histologj^, 

coenzymic  changes  and  corticoid  production.  CIBA  Foundation  CoUoq.  on  Endo- 
crinology 12,  102-121. 
Tamura,  Y.  (1926).  Structural  changes  in  the  suprarenal  gland  of  the  mouse  during 

pregnancy.  Brit.  J.  Exptl.  Biol.  4,  81-92. 
Tanaka,  R.  (1956).  Fluctuations  of  vole  populations  following  the  widespread  synchro- 
nous flowering  of  bamboo-grasses  on  Mt.  Turugi.  Bull.  Kochi  Women's  College  4, 

61-68. 
Tanaka,  R.  (1957).  An  ecological  review  of  small-mammal  outbreaks  with  special  ref- 
erence to  their  association  with  the  flowering  of  bamboo  grasses.  Bull.  Kochi  Women's 

Univ.  Nat.  Sci.  Ser.  5,  20-30. 
Tata,  J.  R.,  Rail,  J.  E.,  and  Rawson,  R.  W.    (1957).   Metabolism  of  L-thyroxine  and 

L-3:5:3'-ti'iiodothyronine  by  brain  tissue  preparations  Endocrinology  60,  83-98. 
Taubenhaus,   M.   (1953).  The  influence  of  cortisone  upon  granulation  tissue  and  its 

synergism  and  antagonism  to  other  hormones.  Ann.  N.  Y.  Acad.  Sci.  55,  666-673. 
Taubenhaus,  M.  and  Amromin,  D.  (1950).  The  effects  of  the  hypophysis,  thyroid,  sex 

steroids,  and  the  adrenal  cortex  upon  granulation  tissue.  J.  Lab.  Clin.  Med.  36, 

7-18. 
Thomas,  L.  (1953).  Cortisone  and  infection.  Ann.  X.  Y.  Acad.  Sci.  56,  799-814. 
Thompson,  D.  Q.  (^1955).  The  I'ole  of  food  and  cover  in  population  fluctuations  of  the 

brown  lemming  at  Point  Barrow,  Alaska.  Trans.  N.  Am.  Wildlife  Conf.  20,  166-176. 
Thorn,  G.  W.,  Jenkins,  D..  and  Laidlaw,  J.  C.  (1953).  The  adrenal  response  to  stress  in 

man.  Recent  Progr.  in  Hormone  Research  8,  171-215. 
Thorn,  N.  A.  (1958).  Mammalian  antidiuretic  hormone.  Physiol.  Revs.  38,  169-195. 
Tobach,  E.,  and  Block,  H.  (1956).  Eft'ect  of  stress  by  crowding  prior  to  and  following 

tuberculous  infection.  Am.  J.  Physiol.  187,  399-402. 
Tyndale-Biscoe,  C.  H.,  and  Williams,  R.  M.  (1955).  A  study  of  natural  mortality  in  a 

wild  population  of  the  I'abbit,   Oryctolagus  cunicuhis   (L.).  New  Zealand  J.  Sci. 

Technol.  B36(6),  561-580. 
Uhrich,  J.  (1938).  The  social  hierarchy  in  albino  mice.  J .  Comp.  Psychol.  25,  373-413. 
Vander  Laan,  W.   P.   (1955).  The  biological  significance  of  the  iodide  concentrating 

mechanism  of  the  thjroid  gland.  Brookhuven  Symposia  in  Biol.  No.  7,  30-39. 
^'ander  Laan,  W.  P.,  and  Caplan,  R.  (1954).  Observations  on  a  relationship  between 

total  thyroid  iodine  content  and  the  iodide  concentrating  mechanism  of  the  th^Toid 

gland  of  the  rat.  Endocrinology  54,  437-447. 
Vander  Laan,  W.  P.,  and  Greer,  M.  (1950).  Some  effects  of  the  hypophysis  on  iodine 

metabolism  by  the  thyroid  gland  of  the  rat.  Endocrinology  47,  36-47. 
\'andeWiele,  R.,  and  Lieljerman,  S.  (1960).  The  metabolism  of  dehydroisoandiosterone. 

In  "Biological  Activities  of  Steroids  in  Relation  to  Cancer"  (CJ.  Pincus  and  E.  P. 

Vollmer,  eds.),  pp.  93-110.  Academic  Press,  New  York. 
Van  Middlesworth,  L.,  and  Berrj',  M.  M.  (1951).  Iodide  metabolism  during  anoxia, 

nephrectomy,  trauma,  avitaminoses  and  starvation  in  the  rat.  .4/?;.  /.  Physiol.  167, 

576-580. 
Verzar,  F.,  and  Vidovic,  V.  (1952).  Action  of  T.S.H.  and  corti.sone  on  uptake  of'-"  I 

by  thyroid  gland  of  hypophysectomized  rats.  /.  Endocrinol.  8,  321-328. 


352  /.  /.   Christian 

"\^etulani,  T.  (1931).  Untersuchungen  iiber  das  Wachstum  der  Saugetiere  in  Abhangigkeit 

von  der  Anzahl  zusammengehaltener  Tiere.  Biol.  Generalis  7,  71-98. 
Visscher,  M.  B.,  and  Halberg,  F.   (1955).  Daily  rhythms  in  numbers  of  circulating 

eosinophils  and  some  related  phenomena  Ann.  N.  Y.  Acad.  Sci.  59,  834-8-49. 
von  Euler,  U.  S.  (1951).  The  nature  of  adrenergic  nerve  mediators.  Pharmacol.  Revs.  3, 

247-277. 
Wallach,  D.  P.,  and  Reineke,  E.  P.  (1949).  The  effect  of  varying  levels  of  thj-roidal 

stimulation  on  the  ascorbic  acid  content  of  the  adrenal  cortex.  Endocrinology  45, 

75-81. 
Waring,  H.   (1935).  The  development  of  the  adrenal  gland  of  the  mouse.  Quart.  J. 

Microscop.  Sci.  78,  329-366. 
Waring,  H.   (1942).  Effect  of  hormones  on  degeneration  of  the  x-zone  in  the  mouse 

adrenal.  /.  Endocrinol  3,  123-131. 
Watt,  L.  J.  (1934).  Frequencj'  distribution  of  litter  size  in  mice.  J.  Mammal.  15,  185-189. 
Weaver,  J.  A.  (1955).  Changes  induced  in  the  thj-mus  and  lymph  nodes  of  the  rat  by  the 

administration  of  cortisone  and  sex  hormones  and  by  other  procedures.  J.  Pathol. 

Bacterial.  69,  133-139. 
Weber,  A.  F.,  McNutt,  S.  H.,  and  Morgan,  B.  B.  (1950).  Structure  and  arrangement  of 

zona  glomerulosa  cells  in  the  bovine  adrenal.  J.  Morphol.  87,  393-416. 
Wexler,  B.  C,  Runfret,  A.  P.,  Griffin,  A.  C,  and  Richardson,  H.  C.  (1955).  Evidence  of 

pituitary  control  of  the  lipid  content  of  the  zona  glomerulosa  of  the  rat  adrenal 

cortex.  Endocrinology  56,  120-123. 
Whitney,  O.  M.,  and  Anigstein,  L.  (1953).  Effect  of  cortisone  on  experimental  rickett- 

sioses  and  on  antibody  formation.  Texas  Repts.  Biol,  and  Med.  11,  561-569. 
Whitten,  W.  K.  (1959).  Occurrence  of  anoestrus  in  mice  caged  in  groups.  J.  Endocrinol. 

18,  102-107. 
Whittle,  P.  (1955).  An  investigation  of  periodic  fluctuation  in  the  New  Zealand  rabbit 

population.  New  Zealand  J.  Sci.  Technol.  B37(2),  179-200. 
WiUiams,  R.  H.,  Jaffe,  H.,  and  Kemp,  C.  (1949).  Effect  of  severe  stress  upon  thyroid 

function.  Am.  J.  Physiol.  159,  291-297. 
Wilson,  H.,  Borris,  J.  J.,  and  Bahn,  R.  C.  (1958).  Steroids  in  the  blood  and  urine  of  female 

mice  bearing  on  ACTH-producing  pituitary  tumor.  Endocrinology  62,  135-149. 
Woodbury,  D.  M.  (1958).  Relation  between  the  adrenal  cortex  and  the  central  nervous 

system.  Pharmacol.  Revs.  10,  275-357. 
Woods,  R.,  and  Carlson,  L.  D.  (1956).  Thyroxine  secretion  in  rats  exposed  to  cold. 

Endocrinology  59,  323-330. 
Wright,  A.,  Jones,  I.  C,  and  Phillips,  J.  G.  (1957).  The  histologj'  of  the  adrenal  gland  of 

the  prototheria.  /.  Endocrinol.  15,  100-107. 
Yamada,  T.  (1959).  Studies  on  the  mechanism  of  hypothalamic  control  of  thyrotropin 

secretion:  Effect  of  intrah^-pothalamic  thyroxine  injection  on  thj'roid  hypertrophy 

induced  by  propjdthiouracil  in  the  rat.  Endocrinology  65,  216-224. 
Yamada,  T.,  and  Greer,  M.  A.  (1959).  Studies  on  the  mechanism  of  h.vpothalamic  control 

of  thjTotropin  secretion:  Effect  of  thyroxine  injection  into  the  hypothalamus  or  the 

pituitary  on  thyroid  hormone  release.  Endocrinology  64,  559-566. 
Yankopoulos,  N.  A.,  Davis,  J.  O.,  Kliman,  B.,  and  Peterson,  R.  E.  (1959).  Evidence 

that  a  humoral  agent  stimulates  the  adrenal  cortex  to  secrete  aldosterone  in  ex- 
perimental secondary  hyperaldosteronism.  J.  Clin.  Invest.  38,  1278-1289. 
Zalesky,  M.  (1934).  A  study  of  the  seasonal  changes  in  the  adrenal  gland  of  Citellus 

with  particular  reference  to  its  sexual  cycle.  Anat.  Record  60,  291-321. 


2.  Endocrines  and  Populations  353 

Zimmermann,  K.  (1937).  Die  markische  Rotenmaus.  Analyse  einer  population.  Markische 

Tierewelt  3,  24-40. 
Zweifach,  B.  W.  (1952).  Functional  deterioration  of  terminal  vascular  bed  in  irreversible 

hemorrhagic  shock.  Ann.  N.  Y.  Acad.  Sci.  55,  370-380. 
Zweifach,  B.  W.,  Shorr,  E.,  and  Black,  M.  M.  (1953).  The  influence  of  the  adrenal 

cortex  on  behavior  of  terminal  vascular  bed.  Ann.  N.  Y.  Acad.  Sci.  55,  626-633. 
Zwemer,  R.  L.  (1936).  A  study  of  adrenal  cortex  morphology.  A7n.  J.  Pathol.  12,  107-114. 
Zwemer,  R.  L.,  Wooton,  R.  M.,  and  Norkus,  M.  G.  (1938).  A  study  of  corticoadrenal 

cells.  Anat.  Record  72,  249-264. 


Author  Index 


N  umbois  ill  italics  indicate  the  pages  on  which  the  complete  references  are  listed. 


Abrams,  G.  D.,  242,  253,  329 

Ackerman,  N.  B.,  233,  328 

Agate,  F.  J.,  Jr.,  244,  328 

Albert,  A.,  232,  328 

Allen,  J.  C,  196,  282,  329 

Allen,  J.  M.,  196,  201,  213,  329 

Alpert,  M.,  194,  196,  329 

Altzuler,  N.,  227,  335 

Amiot,  L.  W.,  97,  187 

Amromin,  D.,  209,  254,  351 

Anderson,  C.  H.,  202,  203,  204,  209,  213, 

329,  338 
Anderson,   E.,   206,   212,    218,   257,   258, 

283,  285,  329,  3U,  348 
Andervont,  H.  B.,  269,  275,  329 
Angevine,  D.  M.,  256,  331 
Angrist,  A.  A.,  208,  210,  346 
Anigstein,  L.,  210,  256,  352 
Antopol,  W.,  282,  329 
Arons,  W.  L.,  233,  328 
Aronson,  S.  M.,  210,  256,  349 
Asling,  C.  W.,  196,  292,  329,  341 
Austin,  C.  R.,  222,  333 
Axelrod,  A.  A.,  238,  343 
Aycock,  W.  L.,  221,  328 
Ayres,  P.  J.,  200,  329 

Bachman,  R.,  193,  329 

Badrick,  F.  E.,  233,  235,  329 

Baeder,  D.  H.,  209,  241,  349 

Bahn,  R.,  212,  218,  283,  329 

Bahn,  R.  C,  203,  206,  207,  208,  209,  212, 

218,  219,  283,  329,  335,  352 
Baker,  B.  L.,  209,  211,  222,  242,  253,  254, 

257,  272,  329,  330 
Baker,  J.  R.,  221,  330 
Baldridge,  G.  D.,  256,  344 
Ball,  W.  C,  Jr.,  204,  335 
Barber,  J.  K.,  204,  330 
Barkalow,  F.  S.,  Jr.,  318,  330 
Baiker,  S.  B.,  229,  330 
Barnett,  S.  A.,  131,  185,  257,  265,  266,  269, 

303,  330 


Barraclough,  C.  A.,  221,  330 

Barry,  M.  C,  249,  330 

Bartter,  F.  C,  201,  202,  203,  204,  330,  344, 

346 
Bascom,  W.  D.,  213,  348 
Bastenie,  P.  A.,  232,  330 
Bates,  R.  W.,  240,  285,  331,  348 
Bauman,  E.  J.,  227,  345 
Beer,  J.  R.,  305,  322,  330,  334 
Beetham,  W.  P.,  Jr.,  223,  226,  339 
Bell,  G.  H.,  223,  224,  225,  330 
Bell,  J.  F.,  225,  253,  320,  340 
Bell,  W.  R.,  215,  334 
Benfey,  B.  G.,  214,  215,  348 
Benua,  R.  S.,  196,  197,  330,  342 
Berry,  M.  M.,  235,  351 
Berson,  S.  A.,  232,  330 
Bethune,  J.  E.,  223,  226,  339 
Beyler,  A.  L.,  292,  330 
Biglieri,  E.  G.,  202,  203,  204,  330 
Bing,  R.  J.,  244,  331 
Birdsell,  J.  B.,  97,  99,  185 
Birmingham,  M.  K.,  216,  SS/ 
Black,  M.  M.,  209,  353 
Bloch,  E.  H.,  206,  244,  331,  344 
Block,  H.,  257,  279,  351 
Boas,  N.  F.,  240,  331 
Bogdanove,  E.  M.,  230,  232,  331,  341 
Bogoroch,  R.,  235,  331 
Bohanan,  E.  H.,  257,  274,  331 
Bois,  P.,  238,  255,  331,  349 
Boring,  W.  O.,  256,  257,  331 
Borris,  J.  J.,  206,  207,  208,  209,  212,  218, 

219,  283,  352 
Botkin,  A.  L.,  233,  331 
Bourliere,  F.,  312,  331 
Bourne,  G.  H.,  193,  194,  331 
Bradlow,  H.  L.,  206,  207,  219,  249,  331 
Braidwood,  R.  J.,  97,  99,  185 
Bridgman,  R.  M.,  242,  253,  329 
Brimblecombe,  R.  W.,  233,  235,  257,  329, 


355 


356 


Author  Index 


Broadbent,  D.  E.,  70,  1S5 

Brobeck,  J.  R.,  213,  345 

Broderick,  C.  B.,  100,  187 

Brown,  D.,  215,  351 

Brown,  R.  Z.,  287,  2S8,  294,  295,  331 

Brownell,  K.  A.,  193,  194,  198,  223,  224, 

227,  228,  261,  341 
Brown-Grant,  K.,  231,  232,  233,  234,  235, 

236,  331 
Bruce,  H.  M.,  239,  331 
Bull,  P.  C,  322,  331,  332 
Bullough,  W.  S.,  198,  210,  269,  332 
Burks,  R.,  215,  351 
Burn,  J.  H.,  225,  332 
Burrill,  M.  W.,  259,  340 
Burrows,  H.,  211,  218,  219,  258,  270,  288, 

289,  292,  332 
Bush,  I.  E.,  206,  207,  208,  212,  219,  332 
Butt,  A.  J.,  209,  349 
Byrnes,  W.  W.,  211,  212,  220,  257,  258, 

259,  309,  332 

Cain,  A.  J.,  195,  332 

Caldwell,  T.  M.,  82, 184 

Calhoun,  J.  B.,  4,  6,  26,  32,  39,  43,  44,  81, 
82,  87,  92,  93,  97,  101,  132,  138,  140, 
148,  150,  161,  165,  168,  185,  273,  289, 
291,  294,  298,  303,  332 

Cannon,  W.  B.,  224,  332 

Cantarow,  A.,  234,  235,  350 

Caplan,  R.,  230,  351 

Carlson,  L.  D.,  228,  234,  342,  352 

Carpenter,  C.  R.,  90,  91,  185 

Casby,  J.  U.,  4,  6,  26,  39,  43,  44,  81,  165, 
185 

(::asper.  A.,  204,  335,  346 

Cater,  O.B.,  195,  •1\:^,332 

Cavazos,  289,  332 

Cervantes,  L.  F.,  100,  /57 

Chang,  M.  C,  288,  332 

Charipper,  H.  A.,  272,  273,  334 

Chart,  J.  J.,  199,  2m,  388 

Cheek,  W.  R.,  215,  2X^,340 

Chitty,  L).,  89,  179,  185,  242,  251 ,  2(\\ ,  264, 
272,  273,  275,  276,  313,  316,  321,  322, 
323,  332,  333 

Chitty,  H.,  222,  242,  261,  2o4,  272,  333 

Christian,  J.  J.,  189,  197,  202,  210,  220, 
221,  222,  225,  226,  242,  244,  246,  247, 
250,  251,  255,  257,  25S,  259,  260,  2(i2, 
264,  265,  266,  267,  268,  269,  27.),  271, 


272,  273,  274,  275,  276,  277,  279,  280, 
281,  282,  285,  286,  287,  288,  289,  290, 

291,  292,  294,  295,  296,  297,  298,  299, 
301,  302,  303,  304,  305,  306,  309,  310, 
311,  313,  315,  317,  319,  320,  321,  322, 
327,  333,  334,  349 

Christianson,  M.,  195,  334 

Chu,  J.  P.,  239,  334 

Clarke,  J.  R.,  242,  251,  263,  272,  281,  291, 

292,  294,  296,  303,  334 
Clayton,  G.  W.,  213,  215,  216,  334,  340 
Cohen,  A.  I.,  206,  331 

Cole,  D.  F.,  204,  334 

Collias,  N.  E.,  96,  186 

Collins,  J.,  234,  235,  240,  341 

Cook,  E.  F,  322,  334 

Cornfield,  J.,  285,  348 

Crabb,  E.  D.,  255,  344 

Crew,  F.  A.,  268,  269,  274,  334 

Critchlow,  B.  v.,  257,  334 

Cronin,  M.  T.  I.,  244,  347 

Crowcroft,  P.,  221,  281,  286,  287,  288,  289, 

290,  291,  292,  294,  308,  334 
Curley,  F.  J,  221,  334 
Cun-ie,  A.  R.,  198,  205,  217,  248,  351 

DahllxMg,  B.  L.,  95,  96,  185 

D'Angelo,  S.  A.,  231,  232,  233,  234,  257, 

258,  272,  273,  334,  350 
Davidson,  C.  S.,  212,  213,  219,  334 
Davidson,  J.  N.,  223,  224,  225,  3,30 
Davis,  D.  E.,  247,  257,  264,  265,  266,  277, 

278,  298,  301,  302,  303,  306,  307,  309, 

310,  311,  320,  333,  334,  335 
Davis,  J.  O.,  202,  203,  204,  335,  352 
Davis,  M.  E.,  210,  259,  335 
Dawson,  J.,  242,  274,  335 
Deane,  H.  W.,  194,  195,  198,  201,  217,  248, 

335,  340,  343 
Deanesly,  R.,  196,  197,  334 
DeBodo,  R.  C,  227,  335 
Deevey,  E.  S.,  Jr.,  97,  185 
Delea,  C.  S.,  202,  203,  204,  330 
Delea,  C,  204,  330 
Delfa,  E.,  239,  343 
Delost,  H.,  196,  336 
Delost,  P.,  196,   197,  218,  219,  220,  283, 

335,  336 
Dempsey,  E.  W.,  195,  198,  217,  238,  248, 

253,  336 
Deeney,  R.,  163,  186 


Author  Index 


357 


Denison,  iM.  E.,  238,  239,  257,  SS6 

Desaulles,  P.,  255,  277,  346 

Desclin,  J.,  219,  336 

Despointes,  R.  H.,  223,  224,  339 

Dixon,  W.  J.,  288,  350 

Dobriner,  K.,  207,  249,  330,  331 

Dorfman,  A.,  209,  336 

Dorfman,  R.  I.,  206,  207,  208,  209,  212, 

218,  219,  250,  253,  259,  336,  347 
Dougherty,  T.  F.,  208,  209,  210,  211,  249, 

250,  254,  255,  256,  336,  348 
Doyle,  D.,  235,  347 
Drager,  G.  A.,  214,  336,  347 
Duff,  W.,  99,  186 
Duncan,  L.  D.,  Jr.,  204,  330 
Duncan,  L.  E.,  202,  203,  204,  344 

Eberhard,  T.,  235,  347 

Egdahl,  R.  H.,  213,  .350 

Ehrich,  W.  E.,  209,  210,  238,  242,  253,  259, 

260,  336,  349 
Eidinoff,  M.  L.,  249,  330 
Eik-nes,  K.,  265,  336 
Eisenstein,  A.  B.,  200,  222,  336,  341 
Elias,  H.,  194,  195,  196,  337 
Eliot,  T.  S.,  244,  344 
Elmadjian,  F.,  223,  226,  227,  244,  337 
Elton,  C,  82,  186,  322,  323,  337 
Emlen,  J.  T.,  271,  281,  284,  285,  287,  288, 

289,  293,  296,  297,  298,  345,  350 
Engel,  F.  L.,  209,  244,  337 
Erachkoo,  ().,  198,  225,  227,  337 
Erickson,  A.  B.,  322,  337 
Ermans,  A.  M.,  232,  330 
Errington,  P.  L.,  298,  317,  337 
Ershoff,  B.  H.,  234,  337 
Evans,  C.  A.,  321,  322,  339,  340 
Evans,  E.  S.,  228,  337,  349 
Evans,  H.  M.,  199,  227,  228,  346 
Everett,  J.  W.,  257,  258,  337,  345 

Fainstat,  T.  D.,  210,  221,  259,  338 
Farrell,  G.  L.,  199,  202,  203,  204,  206,  20s, 

209,  213,  215,  329,  337,  338,  350 
Feldman,  J.  D.,  195,  SS5 
Finerty,  J.  C.,  237,  270,  284,  292,  341,  347 
Finland,  M.,  208,  209,  210,  211,  255,  256, 

343 
Fitch,  H.  S.,  317,  338 
Fleming,  R.  B.,  202,  203,  209,  213,  338 
Flexner,  L.  B.,  195,  338 


FHck,  S.,  309,  338 

Flueckiger,  E.,  232,  338 

Flyger,  V.,  306,  320,  332 

Foenss-Bech,  P.,  209,  213,  344 

FoUey,  S.  J.,  275,  328 

Foot,  E.  C.,  289,  343 

Ford,  E.,  232,  328 

Fortier,  C.,  213,  215,  216,  257,  338,  341 

Frank,  F.,  225,  253,  272,  280,  298,  320,  321, 

322,  323,  338 
Eraser,  F.  C.,  210,  221,  259,  338 
Fredericson,  E.,  47,  186 
Fredrickson,  D.  S.,  227,  338 
Freedman,  H.  H.,  234,  238,  338 
French,  L.  A.,  213,  350 
Fruit,  K.,  21^,345 
Furth,  J.,  206,  212,  218,  283,  329 

Gaddum,  J.  H.,  223,  224,  225,  226,  227, 

338 
Gallagher,  T.  F.,  206,  207,  218,  219,  249, 

330,  331,  338 
Gann,  P.  S.,  203,  204,  330 
Ganong,  W.  F.,  338 
Gaunt,  R.,  199,  201,  338 
Gersh,  I.,  194,  196,  339 
Gerwing,  J.,  236,  237,  339 
Geschwind,  I.  I.,  209,  213,  344 
Gierlach,  Z.  S.,  235,  341 
Giroud,  C.  J.  P.,  199,  200,  202,  339,  350 
Glaubach,  S.,  210,  259,  339 
Glazer,  N.,  103,  186 
c;iick,  D.,  196,  339 
(iodfrey,  G.  K.,  321,  322,  323,  339 
Godwin,  J.  T.,  278,  350 
Goldberg,  R.  C,  232,  339 
Goldfien,  A.,  223,  226,  339 
Goldstein,  M.  S.,  209,  223,  347 
Goodkind,  M.  J.,  204,  335 
Goodman,  H.  M.,  227,  339 
Gordon,  A.  S.,  211,  234,  238,  249,  251,  252, 

254,  256,  272,  273,  334,  338,  339 
Gordon,  R.  S.,  Jr.,  227,  338 
Gorski,  R.  A.,  221,  330 
Gould,  R.  P.,  200,  329 
Gowenlock,  A.  H.,  201,  350 
Grad,  B.,  240,  339 
Grant,  J.  K.,  198,  205,  217,  248,  351 
Gray,  G.,  228,  342 
Gray,  I.,  223,  226,  339 
Gray,  S.  J.,  242,  253,  339 


358 


Author  Index 


Green,  J.  D.,  213,  214,  339 

(iiven,  R.  G.,  225,  253,  320,  321,  322,  83.9, 

340 
Greene,  R.  U.,  259,  340 
Greep,  R.  ().,  195,  198,  201,  217,  232,  248, 

335,  339,  340 
Greer,  M.,  230,  351 

Greer,  M.  A.,  232,  234,  257,  258,  340,  352 
Griffin,  A.  C,  213,  352 
Grollman,  A.,  194,  195,  196,  338,  339 
Gross,  F.,  199,  201,  340 
Gross,  J.,  231,  341 
Grosvenor,  C.  E.,  258,  275,  340 
Guettirigor,  R.  C,  95,  96,  185,  186 
Guillemin,  R.,  213,  215,  216,  334,  340,  348 

Hagen,  P.,  223,  340 

Halberg,  F.,  285,  352 

Halkerston,  I.  D.  K.,  257,  331 

Hall,  C.  E.,  202,  284,  341,  348 

Hall,  C.  S.,  89,  186 

Hall,  0.,  284,  341 

Hallowell,  A.  I.,  98,  186 

Halmi,  N.  S.,  230,  232,  331,  341 

Hamilton,  W.  J.,  Jr.,  308,  317,  320,  341 

Hammond,  J.,  Jr.,  257,  341 

Hamolsky,  M.  W.,  235,  341 

Hardin,  G.,  77,  186 

Harris,  G.  W.,  213,  231,  232,  233,  234,  235, 

236,  331,  341 
Harrison,  R.  G.,  194,  195,  196,  332,  341 
Hart,  P.,  279,  34I 
Hartcroft,  P.  M.,  200,  336,  341 
Hartman,  F.  A.,  193,  194,  198,  223,  224, 

227,  228,  261,  341 
Hatfield,  D.  M.,  317,  341 
Hauser,  E.  A.,  209,  349 
Hayashida,  T.,  209,  213,  344 
Haymaker,  W.,  257,  329 
Hayne,  D.  W.,  81,  186 
Hearn,  W.  R.,  215,  216,  340 
Hellman,  K.,  234,  235,  240,  341 
Hellman,  L.,  218,  219,  338 
Hess,  M.,  237,  239,  270,  284,  292,  S4I,  347 
Hill,  R.  T.,  220,  341,  342 
Hoffmann,  R.  S.,  314,  317,  342 
Hofmann,  F.  G.,  206,  309,  342 
Hollander,  V.  P.,  206,  209,  347 
Holman,  J.,  204,  335 
Holmes,  W.  M.,  196,  197,  342 


Holzhauei',  M.,  223,  224,  225,  226,  227,  338 
Hope,  J.  M.,  223,  226,  227,  244,  337 
Hopsu,  v.,  225,  227,  337 
Horn,  E.  H.,  238,  342 
Housholder,  D.  E.,  215,  216,  3^0 
Howard,  E.,  195,  196,  197,  212,  217,  330, 

342 
Hsieh,  A.  C.  L.,  228,  342 
Hudyma,  G.  M.,  238,  349 
Huggins,  C,  292,  342 
Hughes,  E.,  306,  342 
Hume,  D.  M.,  216,  342 
Hungerford,  G.  F.,  209,  213,  344 
Hurst,  v.,  235,  240,  342 
Hutchinson,  G.  E.,  N.,  77,  186 

Ingalls,  T.  H.,  221,  329,  334,  342 

Ingle,  D.  J.,  208,  209,  211,  253,  257,  329, 

330,  342 
Irving,  J.  T.,  210,  342 
Isler,  H.,  238,  343 
Ivy,  A.  C,  259,  340 

Jacot,  B.,  213,  284,  292,  343 

Jaffe,  H.,  233,  235,  352 

Jameson,  E.  W.,  Jr.,  307,  343 

Jenkins,  D.,  285,  351 

Jennings,  H.  H.,  154,  186 

Jensen,  E.  V.,  292,  342 

Jensen,  H.,  235,  341 

Jensen,  W.  L.,  320,  335 

Jones,  G.  E.  S.,  239,  343 

Jones,  I.  C,  193,  195,  196,  197,  199,  200, 
201,  206,  207,  208,  209,  213,  215,  217, 
218,  219,  254,  261,  283,  334,  343,  347, 
352 

Josimovich,  J.  B.,  195,  S.^5 

Kaljat,  C.,  96,  186 

Kalas,  J.,  241,  349 

Kalela,  O.,  221,  300,  308,  311,  314,  343 

Kalter,  H.,  210,  221,  259,  338,  343 

Kappas,  A.,  218,  219,  335 

Kass,  E.  H.,  208,  209,  210,  211,  255,  256, 

343 
Katonah,  F.,  208,  210,  346 
Katsh,  G.  F.,  256,  339 
Kedda,  L.,  206,  212,  218,  283,  329 
Kelsall,  M.  A.,  255,  344 
Kemp,  C,  233,  235,  352 


Author  Index 


359 


Kendrick,  M.  I.,  208,  210,  211,  255,  S4S 

Keuning,  F.  J.,  211,  255,  344 

Kew,  M.,  99, 186 

Kirschner,  L.,  232,  233,  250,  346 

Kligman,  A.  M.,  256,  344 

Kliman,  B.,  203,  204,  336,  352 

Knigge,  K.  M.,  194,  196,  206,  344,  348 

Knisely,  M.  H.,  244,  344 

Knobil,  E.,  227,  339 

Kraatz,  C.  P.,  239,  344 

Kracht,  J.,  235,  344 

Kraintz,  L.,  232,  233,  250,  346 

Kramer,  J.,  209,  344 

Kuhl,  W.  J.,  Jr.,  232,  344 

Kurlents,  E.,  216,  331 

Kurzok,  R.,  346 

Ladman,  A.  J.,  195,  343 

Laidlaw,  J.  C,  285,  351 

Lamson,  E.  T.,  223,  226,  227,  244,  337 

Lanman,  J.  T.,  195,  344 

Laqueur,  G.  L.,  215,  257,  258,  338,  344 

Larson,  C.  L.,  225,  253,  320,  340 

Leathern,  J.  H.,  288,  344 

Leblond,  C.  P.,  238,  240,  339,  343 

LeMaistre,  C.  A.,  210,  279,  344 

Le  Munyan,  CD.,  257,  258,  260,  274,  275, 

290,  294,  298,  313,  315,  322,  333 
Leopold,  A.  S.,  24,  186 
Leslie,  P.  H.,  242,  261,  264,  272,  333 
Levy,  A.  C,  227,  344 
Li,  C.  H.,  199,  209,  211,  212,  213,  219,  227, 

257,  292,  329,  330,  344,  346 
Lichtlen,  F.,  199,  201,  340 
Liddle,  G.  W.,  202,  203,  204,  330,  344 
Lieberman,  S.,  206,  212,  219,  351 
Lillie,  R.  D.,  253,  344 
Lipner,  H.  J.,  230,  341 
Lipscomb,  H.  S.,  213,  215,  216,  340 
Lipsett,  M.  B.,  218,  219,  338 
Long,  D.  A.,  236,  237,  339 
Long  well,  B.  B.,  206,  347 
Lord,  R.  A.,  278,  344 
Lostroh,  A.  J.,  199,  209,  212,  213,  219,  289, 

344,  345 
Louch,  C,  284,  285,  345 
Louch,  C.  D.,  252,  281,  284,  285,  290,  292, 

293,  294,  296,  297,  304,  305,  345 
Lundgren,  M.  M.,  209,  210,  256,  343 


Lutwak-Mann,  C,  222,  259,  273,  288,  289, 

309,  310,  345 
Lyons,  W.  R.,  209,  213,  344 

McCain,  R.,  24,  186 

McCally,  N.,  202,  203,  204,  209,  213,  329, 

338 
McCann,  S.  M.,  213,  214,  257,  258,  344, 

345 
McCarthy,  H.  H.,  209,  344 
McDermott,  W.,  210,  279,  344 
McDonald,  R.  K.,  215,  345 
Mac  Lulich,  D.  A.,  319,  345 
Mc  Nutt,  S.  H.,  196,  352 
McPhail,  M.  K.,  196,  197,  217,  282,  345 
Mahler,  R.  F.,  201,  350 
Manly,  B.  M.,  257,  330 
Mann,  T.,  288,  289,  345 
Mall,  R.,  306,  342 
Magsood,  M.,  238,  345 
Marine,  D.,  227,  345 
Markee,  J.  E.,  257,  258,  345 
Marmorston,  J.,  215,  348 
Marrian,  G.  F.,  222,  257,  345 
Martin,  E.  P.,  317,  345 
Martorano,  J.  J.,  253,  345 
Mason,  J.  W.,  244,  253,  267,  268,  346 
Meier,  R.,  255,  277,  346 
Meites,  J.,  235,  347 
Melampy,  R.  M.,  289,  332 
Melby,  J.  C,  213,  350 
Menkin,  V.,  254,  346 
Merrill,  P.,  232,  233,  250,  346 
Meyer,  R.  K.,  211,  212,  220,  252,  258,  259, 

284,  285,  305,  330,  332,  345,  350 
Meyers,  V.  M.,  209,  344 
Miller,  R.  A.,  196,  346 
Mills,  I.  H.,  202,  203,  204,  330,  346 
Mirskaia,  L.,  268,  269,  274,  334 
Money,  W.  L.,  232,  233,  234,  237,  250,  346 
Moon,  H.  D.,  199,  209,  212,  213,  219,  227, 

334,  344,  346 
Moore,  W.  W.,  257,  258,  346 
Mora,  P.  M.,  97, 187 
Moreno,  J.  L.,  154,  186 
Morgan,  B.  B.,  196,  352 
Morin,  G.,  228,  346 
Morse,  A.,  195,  335 
Mosier,  H.  D.,  195,  346 


360 


Author  Index 


Moss,  M.L.,  210,  211,3-^6 
Mulinos,  M.  G.,  257,  272,  346 

Nalbanov,  A.  V.,  257,  258,  346 

Nelson,  D.  H.,  206,  207,  208,  346 

Nelson,  D.  M.,  216,  348 

Nelson,  M.  M.,  222,  257,  260,  273,  350 

Nezamis,  J.  E.,  255,  348 

Nichols,  J,  202,  346 

Noble,  R.  L.,  208,  346 

Norkus,  M.  G.,  247,  353 

Norton,  H.  W.,  257,  258,  346 

Nowell,  N.  W.,  215,  216,  346 

Oastler,  E.  G.,  198,  205,  217,  248,  351 
Ochs,  M.  J.,  196,  339 
O'Donnell,  U.  J.,  198,  205,  217,  248,  351 
Odum,  E.  P.,  317,  346 

Palmer,  J.  G.,  216,  348 
Parkes,  A.  S.,  222,  257,  345 
Parmer,  L.  G.,  208,  210,  346 
Parsons,  F.  M.,  292,  34^ 
Paschkis,  K.  E.,  234,  235,  347,  350 
Patric,  E.  F.,  67,  85,  186 
Pauly,  J.  E.,  194,  195,  337,  347 
Pearson,  O.  H.,  218,  219,  338 
Pearson,  O.  P.,  175,  186 
Pechet,  M.  M.,  204,  335 
Peron,  F.  G.,  206,  347 
Perry,  W.  F.,  232,  347 
Peterson,  R.  E.,  203,  204,  335,  352 
Philbrook,  F.  R.,  221,  342 
Phillips,  J.  G.,  195,  352 
Piletta,  P.,  199,  200,  33i> 
Pillsbury,  D.  M.,  256,  344 
Pincus,  G.,  288,  332 
Pitelka,  F.  A.,  307,  319,  323,  347 
Pitt-Rivers,  R.,  236,  237,  339 
Plotz,  E.  J.,  210,  259,  335 
Poindexter,  H.  A.,  257,  347 
Pollard,  M.,  210,  256,  347 
Pomerantz,  L.,  257,  272,  346 

Poore,  W.,  206,  209,  347 

Porter,  J.  C.,  214,  215,  347 

Preble,  E.  A.,  319,  347 

Price,  D.,  288,  289,  345 

Pronove,  P.,  204,  330 

Prudovsky,  S.,  278,  335 

Pruitt,  R.  L.,  215,  348 

Puntriano,  G.,  235,  347 


Raeisaenen,  L.,  198,  227,  337 

Rail,  J.  E.,  229,  230,  231,  238,  347,  351 

Ramey,  E.  R.,  209,  223,  225,  227,  344,  347 

Ramey,  C.  G.,  242,  253,  339 

Rand,  M.  J.,  225,  332 

Randall,  H.  T.,  244,  347 

Ransom,  R.  M.,  221,  330 

Ranson,  S.  W.,  82,  185,  186 

Ratcliffe,  H.  L.,  202,  225,  244,  250,  271, 

333,  347 
Rausch,  R.,  318,  320,  322,  323,  347 
Rauschkolb,  E.  W.,  199,  202,  203,  208,  209, 

213,  338 
Rawson,  R.  W.,  229,  230,  231,  232,  233, 

238,  250,  346,  347,  351 
Read,  C.  P.,  257,  277,  278,  335 
Read,  H.  C.,  196,  197,  217,  282,  345 
Rebell,  G.,  256,  344 
Reed,  C.  A.,  97,  99,  185 
Rees,  R.  J.  W.,  279,  341 
Reichlin,  S.,  231,  232,  233,  234,  235,  236, 

•2-il,331,347 
Reif,  A.  E.,  206,  347 
Reinhardt,  W.  O.,  209,  213,  344 
Reineke,  E.  P.,  237,  238,  345,  352 
Reinhardt,  W.  O.,  292,  329 
Reiss,  J.  M.,  233,  235,  329 
Reiss,  M.,  233,  235,  257,  329,  331 
Rennels,  E.  G.,  214,  270,  284,  292,  34I,  347 
Renzi,  A.  A.,  199,  201,  338 
Retzlaff,  E.  G,  268,  269,  274,  277,  315,  347 
Richards,  J.  B.,  215,  3^5 
Richardson,  H.  C,  213,  352 
Richardson,  J.  A.,  223,  348 
Richter,  C.  P.,  195,  198,  202,  348 
Riesman,  D.,  163,  186 
Riney,  T.,  24,  186 
Ring,  G.  C.,  234,  348 
Rioch,  D.,  257,  258,  344 
Robert,  A.,  255,  348 
Robinson,  H.  J.,  210,  254,  256,  348 
Roby,  C.  C.,  196,  343 
Rogers,  P.  V,  195,  198,  202,  348 
Rosasco,  E.  M.,  80,  85,  187 
Rosemberg,  E.,  257,  258,  285,  344,  348 
Rosenberg,  L.  L.,  349 
Rosenfeld,  G.,  213,  253,  321,  348 
Rothballer,  A.  B.,  214,  348 
Rowe,  F.  P.,  221,  281,  286,  288,  289,  290, 
291,  292,  294,  308,  334 


Author  Index 


361 


246, 
248, 


Royce,  P.  C,  199,  202,  203,  208,  213,  .338 
Rumsfeld,  H.  W.,  Jr.,  214,  215,  347 
Runfret,  A.  P.,  213,  3S3 


Sacks,  J.  G.,  82,  185 

Saffran,  M.,  214,  215,  S^S 

Sakiz,  E.,  218,  348 

Salter,  W.  T.,  230,  348 

Samuel,  L.  T.,  216,  348 

Sandberg,  A.  A.,  216,  3^5 

Sanders,  R.  D.,  96,  186 

Santisteban,  G.  A.,  208,  211,  250,  348 

Sawyer,  C.  H.,  257,  258,  334,  337,  345 

Sayers,  G.,  195,  197,  198,  215,  243, 

248,  348,  351 
Sayers,  M.,  195,  197,  198,  243,  246, 

348 
Scarborough,  H.,  223,  224,  225,  330 
Schaefer,  E.  S.,  143,  155,  162,  186 
Schairer,  M.  A.,  257,  329 
Schally,  A.  V.,  214,  215,  348 
Schapiro,  S.,  215,  348 
Scharrer,  B.,  214,  348 
Scharrer,  E.,  214,  345 
Schindler,  W.  J.,  194,  206,  348 
Schmidt,  L.  H.,  256,  349 
Schmitenloew,  C.  G.,  224,  349 
Schneebeli,  G.  L.,  208,  209,  210,  254, 

256,  336 
Schneirla,  T.  C.,  89,  186 
Schorger,  A.  W.,  96,  186 
Schreiner,  L.  H.,  257,  258,  344 
Schuler,  W.,  255,  277,  346 
Schwartz,  H.  S.,  229,  330 
Scott,  J.  C.,  242,  261,  264,  272,  333 
Scudder,  J.,  244,  349 
Sealander,  J.  A.,  Jr.,  273,  349 
Seifter,  J.,  209,  210,  238,  241,  242,  253, 

260,  336,  349 
Seligman,  A.  M.,  194,  335 
Selye,  H.,  192,  208,  209,  211,  212,  213, 

220,  236,  238,  242,  243,  244,  250, 

254,  255,  256,  257,  258,  260,  261, 

274,  275,  276,  284,  288,  292,  331, 

349 
Share,  L.,  216,  349 
Shaw,  J.  H.,  195,  201,  335 
Shipley,  E.  G.,  211,  212,  220,  257,  258, 

309,  332 


259. 


217, 
253, 
264, 

343, 


259, 


Shipley,  R.  A.,  206,  208,  212,  218,  219,  250, 

259,  336 
Shiras,  G.,  96,  186 
Shorr,  E.,  209,  353 
Shwartzman,  G.,  210,  256,  349 
Sideman,  M.  B.,  209,  213,  344 
Simay,  Kramer,  M.,  209,  344 
Simpson,  M.  E.,  199,  222,  227,  228,  257, 

260,  273,  346,  349,  350 
Simpson,  S.  A.  S.,  200,  329 
Sloviter,  239,  331 

Slusher,  M.  A.,  215,  216,  248,  349 

Smelser,  G.  K.,  240,  349 

Smelser,  J.,  346 

Smith,  A.  L.,  210,  254,  256,  348 

Smith,  E.  K.,  220,  349 

Smith,  J.  D.,  213,  215,  216,  340 

Smith,  R.  W.,  Jr.,  257,  348 

Snyder,  R.  L.,  309,  349 

Sobel,  H.,  215,  348 

SoUman,  T.,  229,  230,  231,  238,  349 

Sonenberg,  M.,  230,  231,  238,  347 

Southern,  H.  N.,  89,  179,  185,  289,  349 

Southwick,  C.  H.,  252,  265,  281,  285,  287, 

288,  289,  293,  294,  295,  296,  297,' 3.50 ' 
Spalding,  M.  H.,  195,  343 
Speirs,  R.  S.,  211,  252,  285,  350 
Spirtos,  B.  N.,  230,  341 
Squires,  W.  L.,  256,  349 
Srebnik,  H.  H.,  222,  257,  260,  273,  350 
Stachenko,  J.,  199,  200,  202,  339,  350 
Stack-Dunne,  M.  P.,  195,  213,  332 
Stadler,  J.  B.,  216,  3^5 
Stanbary,  S.  W.,  201,  350 
Steinberger,  E.,  288,  350 
Stevens,  C.  E.,  234,  350 
Strecker,  R.  L.,  271,  281,  287,  288,  289, 

293,296,297,298,350 
Stewart,  J.  O.,  97,  186 
Stoner,  R.  D.,  278,  350 
Story,  J.  L.,  213,  350 
Sturgis,  S.  H.,  258,  350 
Sundberg,  R.  D.,  211,  350 
Sunderman,  F.  W.,  234,  350 
Swanson,  H.  E.,  228,  229,  234,  350 
Sweat,  M.  L.,  206,  350 
Swift,  E.,  96,  186,  187 
Sydnor,  K.  L..  215,  351 
Symington,  T.,  198,  205.  217.  248,  351 
Szego,  CM.,  292,  330 


362 


Author  Index 


Tait,  J.  F.,  200,  339 

Tamura,  Y.,  196,  198,  351 

Tanaka,  R.,  318,  351 

Tata,  J.  R.,  229,  351 

Taubenhaus,  IM.,  208,  209,  210,  254,  351 

Tenney,  A.,  232,  338 

Tevis,  L.,  Jr.,  24,  186 

Tew,  J.  T.,  233,  331 

Thomas,  L.,  210,  254,  255,  256,  351 

Thompson,  D.  Q.,  318,  351 

Thorn,  G.  W.,  285,  351 

Thorn,  N.  A.,  241,  351 

Timiras,  P.,  235,  331 

Tobach,  E.,  89,  186,  257,  279,  351 

Tompsett,  R.,  210,  279,  344 

Traum,  R.  E.,  231,  232,  233,  234,  257,  258, 

334 
Turner,  C.  W.,  235,  240,  258,  275,  340,  342 
Tyler,  F.  H.,  216,  348 
Tyndale-Biscoe,  C.  H.,  322,  351 

Uhrich,  J.,  271,  351 
Uotila,  U.  U.,  238,  336 

Vander  Laan,  W.  P.,  230,  351 

Van  der  Slikke,  L.  B.,  211,  255,  344 

Vande  Wiele,  R.,  206,  212,  219,  351 

Van  Middlesworth,  L.,  235,  351 

Venning,  E.  H.,  200,  339 

Verzar,  F.,  232,  338,  352 

Vetulani,  T.,  276,  352 

Vidovic,  v.,  232,  352 

Visscher,  M.  B.,  285,  352 

Von  Elder,  C,  231,  331 

Von  Elder,  U.  S.,  223,  234,  236,  352 

Von  Foerster,  H.,  97,  187 

Wagner,  H.  N.,  Jr.,  215,  345 
Walker,  D.  L.,  256,  257,  331 
Wallach,  D.  P.,  237,  352 
Waring,  H.,  196,  197,  352 
Warner,  L.,  244,  344 
Watt,  L.  J.,  309,  352 


Weaver,  J.  A.,  211,  249,  250,  352 

Webb,  W.  L.,  32,  80,  82,  85,  185,  187 

Weber,  A.  F.,  196,  352 

Welch,  A.  D.,  223,  340 

West,  C.  D.,  218,  219,  338 

Wexler,  B.  C,  213,  352 

Whitney,  D.  M.,  210,  256,  352 

Whitten,  W.  K.,  275,  352 

Whittle,  P.,  322,  352 

Whyte,  W.  H.,  163,   187,   198,  205,  217, 

248,  351 
Wiese,  V.  K.,  215,  345 
WiUiams,  R.  H.,  233,  235,  352 
Williams,  R.  M.,  322,  351 
Williamson,  H.  0.,  255,  257,  277,  334 
Wilson,  B.  R.,  210,  256,  347 
Wilson,  H.,  206,  207,  208,  209,  212,  218, 

219,  283,  329,  352 
Wolff,  F.,  232,  339 
Woodbury,  D.  M.,  213,  216,  352 
Woods,  E.  F.,  223,  348 
Woods,  J.  W.,  231,  232,  233,  234,  236,  341 
Woodward,  P.,  213,  345 
Wooton,  R.  M.,  247,  353 
Wright,  A.,  195,  343,  352 

Yalow,  R.  S.,  232,  330 

Yamada,  T.,  234,  352 

Yankopoulos,  N.  A.,  203,  204,  335,  352 

Yatsu,  F.  M.,  202,  203,  209,  213,  338 

Zalesky,  M.,  196,  353 
Zarafonetis,  C.  J.  D.,  241,  349 
Zarrow,  M.  X.,  238,  239,  257,  336 
Ziff,  M.,  232,  344 
ZUeli,  M.  S.,  223,  226,  339 
Zimmerman,  C.  C,  100,  187 
Zimmermann,  K.,  311,  353 
Zippin,  C,  26,  187 
ZuRhein,  G.  M.,  257,  331 
Zweifach,  B.  W.,  209,  244,  353 
Zwemer,  R.  L.,  247,  353 


Subject  Index 


AAF,  216 
Aborigines 

Australian,  99 
ACTH,  197,  200,  209,  214,  220,  232,  243 

assajdng,  248 

basal  release  rate,  215 

A-1  fraction,  203 

endogenous,  249 

exogenous,  249 

inhibiting  the  release,  215 

mice,  237 

rabbits,  237 

rats,  237 

regulation  of  the  release,  213 

release,  216,  217 

release  fraction,  215 

secretion,  237,  258 

secretion  of  ADH,  241 

secretion  of  aldosterone,  202 

sj-nthesis,  216 
Activity 

hyperactivity,  9 

minimal,  9 

normal  level,  16 

normal  nocturnal  period,  16 

24-hour  rhj-thm,  9 

endocrine  glands  of,  192 

evolutionary,  123 
Adaptive  mechanisms,  190 

atrophy,  244 
Adaptive  responses 

measurements  of  the  endocrine,  242 
Adenohypophysis,  214,  233 

arterial  supply,  213 
ADH,  201,  214,  241 
Adirondacks,  55 
Adjustment 

learned,  89 
Adrenal 

androgen,  206 

effect  on  nursing  young,  276 

atrophy,  238 

ascorbic  acid,  248 
depletion,  192 

Callothrix  nrgentata,  196 

cat,  195 

Cerropithent^,  195,  196 


chimpanzee,  195 

cholesterol,  192,  193,  248 

circulation,  195 

Ciiellus  tridecemlineatus,  196 

Cleihriononiys  glareolus,  196 

colobus  monkej',  196 

Colohus  polykomos,  196 

cortex,  191,  192,  193,  217,  245,  246 

hormones,  199 

human  beings,  212 

lipid  vacuoles,  194 

measurements  of  increased  function, 
245 

Microsorex  hoyi,  246 

parenchjina,  195 

Sorex  cinereus,  246 

Sorex  dispar,  246 

Sorex  fumeus,  246 

Sorex  palustris,  246 

secretorj'  activity,  248 

sex  steroids,  219 

sex  steroidal  activity,  218 

sudanophilia,  197 

X-zone,  217 

X-zone  of  mice,  282 

zona  fasciculata,  194 

zona  glomerulosa,  194,  199 

zona  reticularis,  194 

zonation  of,  194 
demeduUation,  228 
cow,  196 
Crocidura,  196 
gland,  192 

effects  of  h\T5ophysectom\-  on,  196 

fetal  zones  of  the,  195 

general  morphology  of,  193 

ground  squirrels,  196 

hamsters,  196 

histologic  measurements,  247 

histology,  195 

humans,  195,  196 

hypertrophj',  238,  246 

lipid  content,  193 

Loris,  196 

Macaca  mulatta,  195 

macaque,  195,  196 

marmoset,  196 


363 


364 


Subject  Index 


Adrenal — continued 

medulla,  193,  198,  223,  224,  245,  246 
hypertrophy,  199 
Microsorex  hoyi,  246 
Sorex  dispar,  246 
Sorex  fumerus,  246 
/Sorex  cinereus,  246 
Sorex  palustris, 
tumors,  199 
Mesocricetus  auratus,  196 
mice,  196 

Microtus  agrestis,  196 
Microtus  arvalis,  196 
monkeys,  195,  196 
Ornithorhynchus,  195 
Orydolagus,  195 
Pan,  195 

Perodicus  potto,  195 
pituitary,  197 
Pilymys,  196 
potto,  195 
primates,  195 
rabbits,  195 

laboratory  rabbit,  195 
rats,  195,  196,  198 
Norway  rat,  195 
wild  rats,  195, 
Rattus  alexaadrinus,  195 
Rattus  norvegicus,  195 
regulation  of  hormones,  213 
reticularis,  198 
samples,  247 
slow  loris,  196 
Sorex  araneus,  196 
starvation,  196 
steroids,  219 
steroid  secretion,  192 

androgenic  metabolites,  219 
synthesis,  219 
Tachyglossus,  195 
transplantation,  21 X 
ungulates,  196 
vascularization,  195 
weight,  192,  245 
factor,  216 
fighting,  264 
variation,  246 
X-zone,  196 
Adrenalectomized  animals,  201 
Adrenocortical  activity 
indices,  21 1 


in  natural  populations,  300 
sudanophilia,  248 
Adrenocortical-gonadal  interrelationships, 

218 
Adrenocorticotropin,   196,    197,   205,   213, 
220,  222 
release,  216 
secretion,  215 

secretion  of  aldosterone,  202,  205 
inaccuracy  of  determining,  308 
Age 

determination  by  teeth,  311 
Alarm  stimulus,  192 
Alberta,  54 
Albino  mice  {see  also  mice) 

trichinosis,  278 
Aldosterone,  199 
release,  217,  243 
secretion 

decrease  in  blood  volume  stimulates, 

204 
and  pulse  pressure,  204 
stimulation    by    decrease    in    blood 

volume,  204 
regulation,  202,  203 
renal  hemodynamics,  204 
stimulation  of,  203 
Aliesterase,  200 
Alley 

emotional  activity,  177 
Alouaita  palliata,  90 
Alpha  individuals,  59 

central,  59 
Amines 

pressor,  225 
Amine  oxidases,  223 
Androgen,  217 

adrenocortical,  219 
cortical,  220 
precursors,  206 
Androgenic  steroids 
house  mice,  220 
man,  220 
rats,  220 
Androstenedione 

gonadotropic  activity,  220 
Animal  {see  also  specific  animal  names), 
239 
laboratory,  262 
.4  ntelioinys    sni  ithii 

inhibition  of  reproductive  function,  318 


Subject  Index 


365 


Antibody  formation,  255,  278 

inhibition,  210 
Antibodies 

stored,  21 1 
Antidiuretic  hormone,  201 
Antigonadotrophic  activity,  211 
Area 

determination,  84 
Ascendency 

dominant,  55 

psychological,  55 
Ascorbic  acid,  217,  237 

adrenal,  216,  248 

factor,  216 
Associations 

intraspecific,  64 
Audition,  38 

Australian  aborigines,  97 
Avoidance 

of  a  strange  field,  17 
Awareness 

of  self,  150 
AWF,  216 

Bainbridge 

cardiac  reflex,  230 
Balance 

electrolyte,  192 

fluid,  192 
Barter  Island,  Alaska,  318 
Basic  group  size,  113,  125 
Basic  number,  3 
Bats    (see    also    generic    names    such    as 

My  Otis),  193 
Behavior,  15,  158 

alterations,  94 

cultural  origin,  124 

exploratory,  175 

genetic  origin,  124 
Behavioral  sink,  93 

development  by  Norway  Rat,  92 
Beta  individuals,  59 
Bivariate  normal  distribution,  20 

function,  4,  39 
Blarina,  29,  30,  31,  32,  36,  38,  52,  61,  64, 
66,  68,  69,  72,  75,  76,  78,  80 

population,  305 
Brain 

subcortical  portion,  15 


British  Columbia 

Indians  of,  99 
Broadbent's  theory,  70,  72 
Brown  fat,  194 

Cages 

"life-space,"  179 
Callothrix  argentata,  196 
Capture 

cumulative  probability,  21 
Carbohydrate-active  corticoids 

suppressive  effects,  209 

Cl9 

steroids,  206,  217,  219 

ketosteroids,  205 
Caribou,  122 
Catch 

comparative,  29,  30,  31,  32 

decline,  56 

Huntington  WildUfe  Forest,  29,  30 

Maine  (1950)  and  Maryland  (1953),  31 

Peromyscus  and  Clethrionomys,  32 
Cattle,  194 
Cells 

inflammatory-,  210 
Census 

30-day,  53 
Central  nervous  system,  191 

integrative  role,  242 

memory  store,  134 
Cercopithecus,  195,  196 
Cervus  nippon,  323 

population  density,  306 
Chadwick  Woods,  76 
Changes 

environmental,  190 

liver,  260 

pancreas,  260 

parotid,  260 

visceral  degenerative,  260 
Chimpanzee,  195 
Chromaffin 

cells,  198 

tissue,  195,  223,  225 
Circulatory  collapse,  244 
Citellus  tridecemlineatus,  196 
Clethrionomys,  27,  30,  31,  32,  33,  34,  35,  36, 
52,  53,  54,  55,  61,  67,  68,  69,  70,  75, 
78,  80,  83,  85,  88,  314,  323 

inhibition  of  growth  and  reproduction, 
316 


366 


Subject  Index 


Clethrionomys — continued 

relationship  between  population  densitj' 

and  reproduction,  310 
reproductive  function,  314 
Clethrionomys  glareolus,  196 
Clethrionomys  rujocanus,  311 
population  density,  308,  313 
territory,  300 

inhibition  of  reproductive  fun(^tion,  318 
Close  confinement 

effects  of,  271 
Codominants,  67 
Cold 

adaptation,  228 
exposure,  234 
Colobus  polyl'omos,  196 
Colony 

compact,  86 
evolution  of,  88 
Howler  monkeys,  90,  92 
Norway  rat,  92 
formation,  87 
stability,  89 
Communication,  38,  125 

low  frequency,  59 
Communication 
reduction,  67 
vocal,  70 
Communication  constant,  133 
Communication  function  (/*),  126 
Community,  2,  8,  32,  35,  49,  55,  74,  78 
Blarina:  Peromyscus,  73 
one-species,  73 
psychological  apex,  79 
small-mammal,  75 
Sorex:  Clethrionomys,  73 
subordinate  member,  78 
Competition,  78 
food  and  social,  271 
social,  262 
"Competitive  exclusion,"  77 
Configuration 

active  rejection  of  new,  179 
physical  nonsocial,  183 
seeking  of  new,  178 
Conformity,  162 
Constellation,  85 
adjoining,  60 

derivation  of  compact  colonies  from,  86 
expected  viability  in,  61 
formation,  57 


individuals  forming,  63 
loosely  knit,  88 

number  of  individuals  forming,  (il 
periphery,  60 
theoretical,  62 
Contact 

between  individuals,  105 
decrease  of  responsive-responsive,  107 
encounters,  106 
frequency,  38,  106,  139 
frequency  of  responsive-responsive,  106 
individual,     106 
refractory,  frequency  of,  105 
responsive-refractory,  103 
responsive-responsive,  103 
social  perception,  136 
variables  determining,  101 
Copulation  pressure 

criticized,  293 
Cortex  {see  also  adrenal  cortex),  72 
adrenocorticotropin,  197 
cellular  hj^perplasia,  213 
fasciculata,  198 
h^-pertrophy,  213 
Cortical  glomerulosa,  216 
Cortical  hormones 

relationships,  212 
Cortical  hyperplasia 

cortical,  218 
Cortical  mass 

increase,  135 
Cortical  stroma 
carnivores,  194 
rodents,  194 
Corticoids 

carbohydrate-active,  220,  245 
secretion,  222 
Corticosteroids 

release,  213 
Corticosterone,  205,  206 
mice,  208 
rats,  208 
Corticotropin  peptides,  200 
Corticotropin-releasing  factor,  214 
Cortisone,  196,  197,  205,  232 

tetratogenic  effects  in  mice,  210 
Cotton  rat,  {see  also  Sigmodon),  323 
Cow,  196 
Creativity,  162 
Crocidura,  196 


Subject  Index 


367 


Crocidura  russula,  197 
Cultural  disturbance,  99 
Cynomys  ludovicianus,  122 
Cytochrome,  230 
Cytochrome  oxidase,  223,  230 

d-gene,  134 

differentiation    in     a    similarity    rank 
hierarchy,  142 

dominant,  128,  141 

recessive,  128,  141 

transformation  of  recessive,  159 
DCA,  205 
DOC,  205 
DOCA,  205 
Deer 

adrenals,  305 

Northern  Wisconsin,  95 

white-tailed,  95 

yarding,  95 
Deer  mice  (see  also  Peromyscus),  322 
Dehydroepiandrosterone,  206,  212 
Density 

actual,  53 

high,  53 

increase,  138 

low,  53 

low  spring,  54 
Density  function,  4,  6,  44,  45,  47 

Cartesian  coordinates,  4 
Deoxycorticosterone,  196,  199 
Diabetes,  241 

Dicrostonyx  {see  also  lemming)  323 
Dicrostonyx  groenlandicus 

population  cycles,  318 
Diestrus,  239 
Differences 

morphologic,  190 
Diffuse  motor  activity  (DMA),  175 
Digestive  organs 

effect  of  stimuli,  253 
Diiodotyrosine,  230 
Disease  resistance 

in  dense  populations,  319 
Distance 

between  neighbors,  44 

methods  of  calculating  data,  44 
Distribution 

spatial,  58,  59 
uniform,  81 


Dog 

adrenal  medulla,  225 

dominant,  265 

hypophysectomized,  233 

release  of  radioiodine,  233 

subordinate,  265 

secretion  of  aldosterone,  203 
Dominance 

psychological,  74,  75,  77,  79 

Effector,  134 
Eimeria  stiedae,  322 
Electrolyte  balance,  201 
ll-Deoxycorticosterone,  205 
ll-Deoxy-17-hydrocorticosterone,  205 
IIB-Hydroxy testosterone,  211 
11-0H4AD,  206,  211 
Elk,  122 
Embrj^o 

resorption  of,  310 
Emotional  activity  alley,  181 
Encounters,  106 
Endocrine 

adaptive  mechanisms,  191 

adaptive  responses,  189 

function 

inhibition,  221 

organs 

relationships,  227 
Endogenous  corticosteroid  seci-etion,  2 1  1 
Environment 

abnormal,  108 

external,  190 

impact  on,  43 

of  all  individuals,  44 

initial  hyperactivity  in  a  strange,  15 

internal,  190,  261 

optimum  uniform  utilization,  60 

shifts,  190 

structured,  11 

two-dimensional,  S 
Environmental  factors,  190 
Eosinopenia,  252 
Eosinophil 

as  index  of  adrenocortical  activity,  211 

counts,  252,  285 

as  index  of  population  density,  284 
Epinephrine,  209,  216 

action,  215 

effects,  223,  224 

secretion,  226,  227 


368 


Subject  Index 


Estradiol,  208 
Estrus,  239 

changes,  258 
Evolution,  51 

cultural,  98,  122 

of  a  new  species  or  genus,  87 

of  compact  colony,  88 

of  a  filtering  device,  134 

probable  cause,  74 

man's  social,  184 

social,  3,  137 

P^acilitation,  151 
Factor 

communication-inhibiting,  112,  139 

contact-Winding,  112,  139 
Families 

human,  100 
F:B  ratio 

monkeys,  206 

mice,  206 

rabbits,  206 

rats,  206 
Feedback  mechanisms 

physiologic,  190 
Female 

asexual,  88 

estrus,  89 
Ferguson  activity  allc}',  S,  18 
Fertility 

density,  290 

female,  239 

male,  239 

mouse,  male,  290 
Fetus 

congenital  defects,  210 

growth  and  development,  259 

prenatal  mortality,  293 

resorption,  95 
Fighting 

as  a  measure  of  social  rank,  267 

as  form  of  stress,  264 
Filter 

Broadbent,  71 

neural,  71,  74 

system,  77 
Food 

limiting  population  growth,  298 


Food 

scarcity,  98 
Food  supply 

effects  on  population  growth,  296 
Frequency 

of  meeting  of  responsive  individuals,  112 

of  meeting  of  responsive-nonresponsive 
individuals,  112 
Frustrations 

optimum,  119 
FSH,  259 
Functions 

interaction,  113 

Gene 

d-,  128 

frequency,  125 

mutant,  125 
Gene  pool,  125 
Gamma  individuals,  59 
Gastrointestinal  tract 

effect  of  stimuli,  253 

integrative  role,  242 
Glomerular  filtration  rate,  201 
Glomerulotropin,  203,  205 

depression,  204 

secretion,  204 
Glucagon,  240 
Glucocorticoids,  245 
Glucose  utilization 

inhibition,  209 
Glj'cogen 

exhaustion,  253 

showshoe  hares,  253 

voles,  253 

levels 

factors  altering,  253 
Glycosuria,  209 
Gonadal  endocrine  cells 

steroids,  219 
Gonadectomy,  219 
Gonadotropin,  217,  219 

inhibition,  211,  220 

production,  211 

secretion 

suppression,  257,  309 
Granulation 

effect  of  corticoidson,  254 


Subject  Index 


369 


Gravidum,  309 
Ground  squirrels 

thirteen-lincd,  196 
Group  dynamics 

formulation,  101 
Group  size 

basic,  3 

change,  125 

effect  on  reproduction,  91 

optimum,  2 

saltatorial  changes  in  the  basic,  122 

satisfaction  and  frustration  as  a  function 
116 
Growth,  254 

effects  of  increased  population  density, 
276 

house  mice,  299 

population,  1S9 
Guinea  pig 

harderian  glands,  240 

hydrocortisone,  236 

medullary  hypertrophy,  227 

pituitary-adrenocortical  activity,  273 
Guinea  pig 

pneumococcal  infection,  256 

streptococcal  infection,  256 

thyroid  activity,  236 

tuberculosis  infections,  256 

Habitat,  42 

marginal,  45 

one-dimensional,  8 

unstructured  one-dimensional,  9 

unsuitabilitj',  SO 
Hamster,  196 

golden,  194 

poliomj-elitis,  256 

sudanophilia,  248 
Hare  (see  also  Lepus),  319,  323 

mortality,  322 

population  density  and  parasitism,  322 

relationships   between   population   den- 
sity and  reproduction,  310 
Harvest  mice,  6 
Hematopoiesis,  251 
Hibernating  gland,  194 
Hierarchical  situation,  263 


Hierarchy  formation 

reductions  in  velocity  of,  153 
Histamine,  216 
Homeostasis 
circulator}',  192 
electrolyte,  202,  205 
fluid,  202,  205 
Home  range,  20,  25,  36,  38,  42,  164 

behavioral     origins     of     the     bivariate 

normal,  8 
bivarate  normal,  4 
center,  4,  23,  39,  42,  43,  47,  57,  58,  72 

actual,  64 

"ideal"  interval  between,  45 

optimum  interval,  51 

uniform  distribution,  59 
clumping,  67 
contracted,  54 
enlargement,  76 
expansion,  68,  79 
fixed,  77 

inhibition,  53,  78,  80 
intraspecific  differences  in  size  of,  75 
mutual  inhibition  of,  79 
overlapping,  67 
periphery,  51 
reduction,  70 
relative,  165 

relative,  of  constellation  members,  166 
schematic 

for  Blarina,  65 

for  Peromycus,  65 

for  Pitymys,  66 
sigma,  5,  6 
size,  53 

social  inhibition,  70 
travel-path,  24 
Homo  sapiens,  57 
Hormone 

antidiuretic,  241 
anti-inflammatory,  208 
fascicular  action,  208 
regulation  of  secretion  of  the  fascicular, 
213 
Host  resistance 
decrease,  210 
effect  of  crowding,  279 
infectious  disease,  279 
parasites,  279 


370 


Subject  Index 


House  mice  {see  also  mice,  mouse,  Mus), 
140,  148,  180,  197,  251 
ACTH  and  gonadotropin  relationship, 

260 
adrenals,  206 
adrenal  cortex,  217,  282 
adrenal  weight,  266 
albino,  269 
birth  rates,  292 
competition,  271 
competition  for  food,  297 
crowding  mortality,  280 
decrease  in  reproduction,  286 
food,  296 
growth,  276 
inanition,  272 
inhibited  growth,  299 
intra-uterine  mortality,  221 
litter  survival,  294 
nests,  294 

pituitary-adrenocortical  activity,  273 
resorption  of  the  embryos,  221 
responses  to  increased  population  den- 
sity, 281 
responses    to    sociopsychologic    factors, 

269 
self-limited  populations,  299 
social  pressure,  288 
spermatogenesis,  288 
splenic  hypertrophy,  272 
trichinosis,  278 
wild,  269 
wild-stock,  280 
Howler  Monkeys,  90,  121 
Humans,  196 
Humans 

dominant,  132 

hyi)othyroidism,  238 

responses    to    sociopsychologic    factors, 

269 
thyroid,  232,  236 
Hum  field,  41,  50 
Humoral  substance,  16 
Huntington  Forest,  27,  61,  82 
Hyaluronidase,  209 
Hydrocortisone,  205,  236 
Hydrocortisone 
cats,  206 
ferrets,  206 
guinea  pigs,  206 


humans,  206 

mice,  20^ 

monkeys,  206 

rats,  208 

sheep,  206 
HjTJeractivity,  175 

in  a  strange  environment,  16 
Hyperglycemia,  209,  224,  226 
Hyperthyroidism,  239 
Hj^pertrophy 

cortical,  216 
Hypoglycemic  shock,  320,  321 
Hypophysectomy 

secretion  of  aldosterone,  202,  203 
Hypothalamic  centers,  216 

Indians 

Kunghit  Haida,  99 
Individuals 

alpha,  59,  61,  165 
beta,  59,  165 
gamma,  59,  165 
Infection 

resistance  to,  256 
Inflammation 

effect  of  corticoids  on,  254 
Inflection  point 

arithmetic,  121 
Inhibitory  influence,  38 
Input 

rates,  56 
Insulin,  240 
Intensity  of  action,  112 
Interaction 
constant,  115 
frequency,  108 
frustrating,  119 
intensity,  115 
maximum  frequency,  114 
positively  affective,  112 
refractory,  109 
responsive,  109 
variables  determining,  101 
Interconstellation  matrix,  60,  68 
Intraspecific  competition,  263 
Intrauterine  mortality,  259 
Invasion 

induced,  80 
Iodine,  230 
Iraq,  99 
Islets  of  Langerhans,  240 


Subject  Index 


371 


Isolation 

spatial,  69 
Isolation  cage,  17 

Jarmo  site,  99 

Kendall's  compound  A,  205 
B,  205 

E,  205 

F,  205 

Lactation 
failure,  260 
inhibition,  294 
Lemming     (see     also      Dicroslonyx     and 
Lemmus),  320 
populations 

cj'clic  decline,  319 
mortality,  322 
population  cycles,  318 
Lemmus,  305,  323 
Lemmus  trimucronalus 

population  cycles,  318 
Lepus  (see  also  hare),  323 
Lepus  americanus 

relationships   between   population   den- 
sity and  reproduction,  310 
Life-space  quadrants 

shifts  into  the  second  and   fourth   by 
rats,  159 
Lipid  mobilizing  factor,  241 
Lipogenesis,  209 
Litter  mortality 
factors  causing,  295 
mice,  294 
size,  239 
survival 

effect  of  increasing  density  on,  295 
effect  of  nests  on,  295 
factors,  294 
mice,  293,  294 
voles,  293,  296 
Liver,  260 

glycogen,  252 
LMF,  241 
Loris,  196 
Lymph  nodes 

weights,  249,  250 
Lymphocytes 

as  indices  of  adrenocortical  activity,  21 1 


criteria  of  stress,  251 

counts,  251 
Lymphocytolysis,  211 
Lymphocytopoiesis 

depression,  211 
Lymphocytopoiesis 

depression,  211 
Lymphoid  organs 

nucleic  acid  content  of,  255 
Lymphoid  tissue 

growth,  250 

involution,  250 

Macaca  mulatia,  195 
Macaque,  196 
Maine,  32,  54 
Male 

dominant,  89 

rats,  pansexual,  162 

reduction  of,  90 

territorial,  161 
Mammals  {see  also  specific  names) 

adrenalectomized,  208 

European,  196 
Man  {see  also  human)  121,  173,  194 

anestrus,  220 

basic  numbers  for,  97 

corticosteroid  production,  268 

ovarian  atrophy,  220 
^Marking 

with  red  dye,  76 
Marmoset,  196 
Marmota,  193 

relationships  between  population  den- 
sity and  reproduction,  310 
Marmota  monax,  122 
Maryland,  32 

Montgomery  County,  69 
Meadow  mouse  (see  Microtus) 
Medulla  {see  also  adrenal) 

adrenal,  198 

basophilic  granules,  198 

chromaffin  cells,  198 
Medullary  hyperplasia 

mice,  198 

ungulates,  198 
Mesocriceius  auratus    {see  also   hamster), 

194,  196 
Metestrus,  239 


372 


Subject  Index 


Mice  {see  also  house  mouse,  AIus,  white- 
footed  mice,  mouse),  26,  148,  170,  193, 
196,  19S,  212,  218,  222,  257,  264,  277, 
293, 309 

adrenals,  200 

adrenal  weights,  246 

adrenocortical  responses,  270 

albino,  264 

androgen,  221 

anestrus,  220 

antibody  formation,  278 

blood  level  of  TSH,  237 

corticosterone,  236 

Coxsackie  infections,  256,  257 

decline  in  birthrate,  291 

during  pregnancy,  196 

effect  of  new  environment,  265 

effect  of  overcrowding,  269 

effect  of  population  density,  274 

effect  of  social  pressures,  268 

eosinophil  counts,  252 

fighting  social  competition,  287 

gonadotropins,  309 

harderian  glands,  240 

hierarchy,  265 

hypoglycemic  shock,  321 

hypophysectomized,  212 

inanition,  222 

inbred,  274 

inbred  albino,  273 

laboratory,  196 

lactation,  274 

medullary  hypertrophy,  227 

method  of  inducing  an  inflammatory 
response,  277 

nursing,  210 

ovarian  atrophy,  220 

ovaries,  220 

pituitary-adrenocortical  activity,  273 

pneumococcal  infection,  256 

pregnant,  210 

preputial  glands,  292 

radiothyroidectomized,  239 

release  of  iodine,  238 

reproduction,  274 

reproductive  suppression,  275 

resistance  to  infection,  277 

shock  death,  225 

social  hierarcy,  264 

sociopsychologic  interactions,  280 


sjjlenic  hypertrophy,  242,  273,  274 

streptococcal  infection,  256 

susceptibility  to  trichinella,  277 

testes,  238 

thymus,  283 

thj'roid  activity,  235 

trichinosis,  257 

tuberculosis,  257,  279 

tuberculosis  infections,  256 
Microsorex  hiyi,  193,  246 
Microtus  (see  also  voles),  77,  301,  305,  323 

hypoglycemic  shock,  320 

natural  populations,  303 

relationships   between   population   den- 
sity and  reproduction,  310 

"shock  disease",  225 
Microtus  agrestis,  196,  251,  273,  323 

behavior,  222 

estrous  pattern,  222 

inhibition  of  growth  and  i('])roduction, 
316 

number  of  births,  222 

respons(>s  to  increased  pf)pulation  den- 
sity, 281 

sociopsj-chologic  factors,  264 

splenic  hypertrophy,  273 
Microtus  arvalis,  196,  218 

confined  populations,  272 

crowding  mortality,  280 

sex  accessories,  219 
Microtus  californicus,  323 

reproduction,  317 
Microtus  montanus,  305,  316,  317,  320,  323 

population  density,  303,  314 

population  fluctuation,  303 
Microtus  ochrogaster 

reproduction,  317 
Microtus  oeconomus 

population  cycles,  318 
Microtus  orcadensis 

nucleus  pulposus  enlargement,  261 

sociopsychologic  factors,  264 

splenic  hyjjertrophy,  273 
Microtus  pcnnsylvanicus,  323 

adrenocortical-densitj'  relationships,  304 

adrenal  weight,  285 

body-adrenal  relationships,  247 

effect  of  increased  population  density, 
284 


J 


Subject  Index 


373 


lice,  322 

litter-size,  SOS,  317 
population  fluctuations,  304 
productive  cycle,  319 
responses  to  increased  population  den- 
sity, 281 
Migrations 

lemming,  86 
Milk  let-down  reflect,  2r)S 
Monkey  (see  also  Rhesus  monkey),  194 

brucellosis,  256 

colobus,  196 

corticoids,  268 

malaria,  256 
Moose,  122 
Mortality 

alterations,  94 

effect  of  crowding  on,  279 

immediate  cause,  323 

increase,  323 

in  dense  populations,  319 

mass,  322 

of  young 
factors,  321 

prenatal,  315 
Motor  activity,  16 
Mt.  Desert  Island,  33,  53 
Mouse  (see  also  mice),  196,  170,  175 

adrenalectomized  laboratory,  201 

adrenal  gland,  196 

adrenal  weight,  197 

albino,  280 

copulation  pressure,  289 

crowding  mortality,  280 

development  of,  196 

dominant,  264 

fertility,  288 

fights,  296 

male  fertilit\-,  289 

Xoninovolution  of  X-zone,  258 

puberty,  197 
Mouse  (see  also  Clethrionomys) 

red-backed,  27,  52,  54 

subordinate,  264 

tundra,  318 

velocity  in  a  hierarchy,  149 
Mule  deer,  122 

does,  24 

fawns,  24 

males,  24 


Mus  (see  also  mice  and  mouse) 

inhibition  of  growth  and  reproduction, 
316 
Muskrat  (see  also  Ondatra),  323 
endocrines,  305 

relationships  between  population   den- 
sity and  reproduction,  310 
reproduction,  317 
reproductive  organs,  305 
Mus  musculus  {see  also  mice  and  mouse), 
196,  217 
medullar}^  hypertrophj^  227 
responses  to  increased  population  den- 
sity, 281 
Myotis,  193 
Myotis  grisecens,  122 
Myotis  lucifugus,  122 
Myotis  yiunanensis,  122 
Myotis  velifer,  122 

Natural  populations,  323 
Natural  selection,  122 
Neighbors 

contacting,  45,  46 

distance  between,  50 

number  of,  51 

perception  of,  51 

signal  field,  47 

sign  field,  47 
Nerves 

postganglionic  sympathetic,  22:] 

splanchnic  sympathetic,  223 
Nervous  system 

sympathetic,  223 
Neurohypophysis,  214,  241 
Neutrophil 

count,  252 
New  York,  33,  54 
New  Zealand,  322 
Niche 

characterization,  77 

l)rimary  component  of,  77 

specializations,  74 
NIH  Emotional  Activit.y  Alley,  17 
Norepinephrine,  209 

effects,  223,  224 

secretion,  226,  227 

source,  225 
North  American  Census  of  Small  Mam- 
mals, 26 


374 


Subject  Index 


Norwa}^  Rat  (see  also  rat,  Rattus),  87,  180, 
195,  320,  323 

adrenalectomized,  202 

adrenal  weight,  266,  301 

behavioral  sink  development  by,  92 

body-adrenal  relationships,  247 

domesticated,  163,  175 

inhibition  of  reproduction,  307 

intra-uterine  mortality,  221 

laboratory,  195 

relationships  between  population   den- 
sity and  reproduction,  310 

reproductive  depression,  291 

resorption  of  the  embryos,  221 

responses    to    sociopsychologic    factors, 
269 

rural  population,  302 

social  competition,  273 

sociopsj'chologic  interactions,  280 

subordinate  males,  265 

wild,  179,  183,  195,  265 
Number 

total  of  individuals,  110 

Oak,  64 

Ondatra  (see  also  muskrat),  323 

relationships  between  population   den- 
sity and  reproduction,  310 
Ondatra  zibethica 

reproduction,  317 
Open-field  emotional  behavior,  89 
Orders,  122 
Organ 

isolated,  191 
Ornithorhyn  chics,  195 
Oryciolagvs,  193,  323 
Os  penis 

stimulation,  212 
Ovary,  194 
Ovaries 

androgens,  220 

Pan,  195 
Pancreas,  260 
Pancreatic  islets,  240 
Pantothenic  acid 

deficient,  222 
Parasitism,  322 
Parotid.  260 


Particle 

basic,  2 

general  classes,  3 
Partner 

choosing  of,  154 

data  regarding  the  choosing  of,  156 
Perception,  89,  139 
Perodicus  potto,  195 

Peromyscus,  29,  30,  31,  32,  33,  34,  35,  36, 
52,  53,  54,  55,  61,  64,  66,  67,  68,  69, 
70,  75,  76,  77,  78,  79,  80,  88,  323 
Peromyscus  boylii 

inhibition  of  reproduction,  307 
Peromyscus  leucopus 

pituitary-adrenocortical,  273 

population,  305 
Peromyscus  maniculatus 

inhibition  of  reproduction,  307 

lice,  322 

productive  cycle,  319 
Personality,  158 
Phagocytosis 

inhibition,  210 
Physiologic  adaptation 

generalized  effects,  254 
Physiology 

classical,  184 

evolution,  60 

reproductive,  95 
Pigs,  194 
Pine,  64 
Pineal,  204 

Pipistrellus  (see  also  bat),  193 
Pitressin,  214,  232 
Pituitary 

anterior,  197,  220 

antidiuretic,  241 

gonadotropins,  257 

hormones,  218 

lipid  mobilizing  factor,  241 

posterior,  241 

secretion  of  aldosterone,  203 
Pituitary-adrenocortical  exhaustion,  226 

tumors,  218 
Pitymys,  32,  52,  64,  66,  69,  70,  76,  196 
Pitymys  subterraneus,  196,  197 
Population,  189 

collapse,  314 

densities,  190,  238 

fertility,  290 


Subject  Index 


375 


fixed  size,  263 

food-limited,  298 

freely  growing,  281 

individuals  of,  103 

limited  size,  280 

methods  for  estimating  natural,  305 

natural,  190,  262,  300,  306 

physiologic  adaptation,  261 

reproductive  function,  306 

resident,  56 

self-limited,  298 

size,  240 

uniforml}'  distributed,  58 
Population  density,  226,  262,  287,  305 

adrenocortical     and     reproductive     re- 
sponses, 269 

effect  on  adrenocortical  function,  281 

endocrine  responses,  263 

index  of  relative,  305 

inflammation,  276 

resistance  to  infection,  276 

schematic  summary,  326,  327 
Potassium,  204 

excretion,  201 

levels,  204 

level  of  body,  202 

secretion  of  aldosterone,  204 
Potto,  195 

Preconstellation  phase,  58 
Pregneninolone,  210 
Preputial  development 

adrenal  androgens,  292 

stimulating,  292 

stimulation,  212 
Probability 

formulas,  144 
Pi(A),  144 
PiW,  144 

tenninating  trips,  12 

vacillating  at  the  termination  of  trips,  18 
Proestrus,  239 
Psychology 

classical,  184,  185 

Rabbit,  193,  323 

adrenalectomized,  236 
corticosterone,  236 
domestic,  236 
embryo 

resorption,  239 


parasitism,  322 

release  of  radioiodine,  233 

thyroid,  232 

thyroid  activity,  235 

thyroidectomy,  239 

thyrotoxicosis,  235 

wild,  235,  236 
Radial  distance,  623 
Radioiodine 

release  in  domestic  rabbits,  236 

release  in  wild  rabbits,  236 

uptake,  232 
Rat   (see  also  Norway  rat,  Rattus),   138, 
159,  182,  194,  196,  212,  218,  309 

ACTH  and  gonadotropin  relationship, 
260 

adrenals,  201,  206 

adrenal  atrophy,  237 

adrenal  cortex,  200 

adrenalectomized,  253 

adrenalectomized  laboratory,  201 

adrenal  hypertrophy,  237 

adrenal  weight,  302 

adrenocortical  secretion,  222 

androgen,  221 

anestrus,  220 

albino  Osborne-Mendel  strain,  181 

blood  level  of  TSH,  237 

chorionic  gonadotropins,  220 

corticosterone,  236 

domesticated  albino  strain,  171 

domesticated  Norway,  8 

effect  of  temperature  changes  on,  274 

gonadectomized,  219 

gonadotropins,  212,  309 

growth,  210 

harderian  glands,  240 

house,  212 

hypertrophy  of  medulla,  227 

hypophysectomized,  216,  233,  254 

hypothalamic-hypophyseal-thyroidal  re- 
lationships, 233 

hypothyroid,  234,  239 

inanition,  272 

in  a  strong  environment,  303 

inbred,  274 

infant,  210 

length  of  estrous  cycle,  238 

method   of   inducing   an   inflammatory 
response,  277 


376 


Subject  Index 


Rat — continued 

newborn,  210 

Nonvay  (see  Norway  rat) 

Osborne-Mendel,  17 

ovarian  atrophy,  220 

pituitarj^  236 

pituitary-adrenocortical  activity,  273 

pneumococcal  infection,  256 

postparturitional  loss,  221 

pregnant,  210 

release  of  iodine,  238 

splenic  hypertrophj^  274 

streptococcal  infection,  256 

submaxillary  glands,  240 

subordinate,  303 

testes,  239 

of  immature,  238 

thiouracil  treatment,  239 

thyroid,  232 

thyroid  activity,  235,  237 

thyroidectomized,  228 

tuberculosis  infection,  256 

uptake  of  radioiodine,  233 

white,  237 

wild,  195 
Rat  embryos 

action  of  estrogens,  259 
Rat  society,  161 

velocity  in  high-density,  168 

velocity-rank  relationships  in,  168,  169 
Rattus  (see  rat,  Norway  rat) 
Rattus  alexandrinus,  195 
Rattus  norvegicus,  195 

adrenalectomized,  202 

medullar}'  hypertrophy,  227 

relationships   between   population   den- 
sity and  reproduction,  310 
Recapture  radii,  5,  6 
Receptor,  50 
Refractory  period,  49,  141,  153,  176 

duration,  103,  112 

frustrating,  110 

frustrating  type,  131 

frustrating  producion,  112 

increasing,  130 

satisfaction  producing,  112 

time  spent  in  satisfying,  112 
Refractory  state 

maximal,  109 

optimal,  109 


Regression  line,  81 

Reichstein's  compound  S,  199,  205 

Reithrodontomys,  6 

Rejection 

psychological,  183 
Relationships 

spatial,  64 

summary  of  presumed,  137 
Relative  densitj',  78 
Relative  probability,  45 
Removal  study 

Chadwick  Woods,  Montgomery  Count}', 
Maryland,    29 

Rich  Lake  Island,  New  York,  27 
Removal  trapping 

observed  data,  52 
Renal  nephron 

tubular  cells,  201 
Reproduction,  190,  286-291 

evaluating  the  effects  of  densit}'  on,  308 

inhibition,  309 

mice,  female,  291 

mice,  male,  286 

sexual,  133 

suppression,  222,  257 
Reproductive 

inhil)ition,  220 

function,  292 
evaluation,  306 

inhibited  by  population  density,  292 
inhibition,  323 

organs 

accessory  weights,  287 
Reserpine,  225 
Response 

avoidance,  93 

degree,  192 

endocrine,  191 

inflammatory,  210 

intensity,  151 

neural,  191 

neuroendocrine,  191 

nonspecific,  192 

vascular,  191 
Response-evoking  capacities,  143,  146 
Response-evoking  capacity 

S 

behavioral  origin,  140 

circumplex   depiction   of   behavior   and 
personality  superimposed  upon,  157 


' 


Subject  Index 


377 


circumplex,  155 
formulae,  128,  1-13 
S'-^\  143 
S^^\  143 
SjA,  128 
Si^^',  128 
S^'^),  143 
S('),  143 
Responsive  state,  140 
Rhesus  monkey  (see  also  monkey).  267 
hj'drocortisone,  236 
responses    to    sociopsychologic    factors, 

269 
thyroid  activity,  236 
Rodents  (see  also  specific  categories),  212 
Runaways 

underground,  76 

Salivary  glands,  260 

Sanctions,  151,  163 

Scent,  38 

Schaefer's  autonomy,  143 

Schaefer's  control,  143 

Schaefer's  hostility-rejection,  128,  143 

Schaefer's  love-acceptance,  128,  143 

Serotonin,  216 

Sex  accessories,  238 

involution,  218 
Sex  steroids,  206,  217 
Sheep,  194 
Shock,  192,  243,  244 
"Shock  disease,"  226,  280,  320 
Shrews  (see  also  Sorex  and  Blarina),  26,  27, 
75,  193,  197 

long-tailed,  246 

population,  305 

short-tailed,  305 
Sight,  38 
Sigmodon  {see  also  Cotton  rat),  323 

litter  size,  317 

relationships   Ix^twecn   })o})ulation   den- 
sity and  reproduction,  310 
Sign,  47 
Sign  field,  49 
Signal,  53,  57,  167 

detection,  41 

inhibitory,  68 

intensity,  48,  49 

learning  of,  39,  50 

neural,  78 


noxious,  78 

simultaneous,  48 

sound,  41 
Signal  field,  47,  49 
Sika  deer,  323 

mass  mortality,  320 

population  density,  306 
Size 

basic  group,  86 

effect  on  response-evoking  capacity,  130 
Slow  loris,  196 

Snowshoe  hare  (see  also  hare),  253 
Social  behavior,  128 

more  effective,  136 
Social  dominance,  74 

adrenal  activity,  267 

changing,  89 
Social  factors 

adrenocortical  functions,  236 

interspecific,  77 

reproductive  functions,  263 
Social  groups 

budding  off,  121 
Social  hierarchy 

evolution,  70 
Social  interaction,  133 

basic  processes,  116 

consequences  and  examples,  148 

frequency  of  satisfactory,  117 

frustration  from,  110 

as  a  function  of  density,  117 

heightened  frequency,  95 

intensity,  152 

model,  101 

optimum  satisfaction,  118 

and  refractory  period,  117 

satisfaction  from,  110 

withdrawal  from,  173 
Social  phenomena,  3 
Social  pressure 

endocrine  responses,  263 
Social  rank,  64 
Social  relations 

instability,  67 
Social  response 

blocking,  104 

mechanism,  104 
Social  subordination 

interspecific,  67 


378 


Subject  Index 


Social  system,  3,  4 

semiclosed,  100 
Social  withdrawal,  145 
Society 

human,  184 

rat,  161 
Sodium:  potassium  ratio 

serum,  204 
Sodium  retention,  201 
Sorex,  {see  also  shrew),  27,  29,  31,  32,  36, 

'  -  38,  52,  61,  68,  69,  70,  72,  75,  77,  78 
/Sorex  araneus,  196,  197 
Sorex  cinereus,  193,  246 
Sorex  dispar,  193,  246 
Sorex  fumeus,  193,  246 
(Sorex  palustris,  193,  246 
Soricidae    {see    also    shrew,    Sorex,    and 
Blarina) 

adrenal,  246 
Space 

general  theorj-  of  use,  34 

interspecific  and  intraspecific  use,  34 

social  use,  2 

theoretical  conceptualization  of  use,  70 
Spatial  equilibrium,  57 
Species,  3 

alpha,  36,  37 

l)eta,  36,  37 

distance  between  neighbors,  42 

dominant,  36,  52,  67,  68,  78 

incipient,  74 

rf^lationship  between  two  dominant,  52 

removal  captures,  55 

socially  dominant,  55,  57 

subordinate,  36,  38,  52,  61,  67,  68,  78 

survival,  122 
Spermatogenesis, 

decrease,  258 
Spleen 

house  mice,  251 

h^'pertrophy,  251 

Microtus  agrestis,  251 

volves,  251 

weights,  249,  251 
Splenic  hypertrophy,  271 
Standard  area,  5 
State 

nonresponsive  P,  140 
Stations 

feeding,  95 


Status 

subordinate,  68 
Steady  state 

evolutionary,  108 
Steroid  hormones,  218 
Steroids,  248 

sex,  258 

site  of  action,  216 

urinary,  249 
Stimuli 

alarming,  212,  215 
Stimuli 

auditor}^  70 

emotional,  243 

conditioned,  75 
Strange  object  reaction,  89 
Strange-object  response,  179 
Stress,  88,  193,  196,  213,  235 

generalized  state,  182 

resulting  from  group  size  change,  118 
Stressful  state 

amount  of  time  spent,  112 
Stressors 

nonspecific,  119 
Stretch  receptors 

secretion  of  aldosterone,  204 
Strife 

intraspecific,  262 
Sudanophilia,  248 
Supraopticohypophyseal  tract,  214 
Survival 

threshold  of,  132 
Sylvilag^is  {see  also  rabbit),  193 
SjTnpathetic  nervous  system 

ganglia,  198 
System 

Iwpothesized  communication,  58 

two-species,  36 

Tachyglossus,  195 
Target  diameter 

determined  by  variabihty  of  l^diavioral 
traits,  140 

genotype,  140 

hj'pothesized  divergence,  132 

of  individual,  110 

variability,  127 
Territory,  263 

of  male  mice,  287 


Subject  Index 


379 


Testis 

interstitial  cells,  194 
Testosterone,  208 
Thetas 

satisfaction  and  frustration,  131 
Thiouracil,  237 

Thymicolymphatic  system,  249 
Thymus,  283 

cortex,  249 

involution,  208,  211,  249,  250 

medulla,  249 

weights,  249 
Thja-ocarotid  artery,  204 
Thyroglobulin,  230 
Thyroid 

activity 

factors  regulating,  231 
index,  231 
mice,  235,  236 
NaCl  intake,  238 
rabbit,  236 
rat,  235,  236 

gland,  191,  228 

honnone 
actions,  229 
formation,  230 

inhibition,  236 

responses  to  external  stimuli,  234 
Thyroidal-gonadal  interrelationships,  238 
Thyroidectomy,  228,  234 
Thyrotropin,  231 

inhibition,  236 
Thyroxine,  228,  229,  231 

effects,  230 
Tonus 

vascular,  224 
Trap,  20,  37 

"Havahart,"  184 

probability  of  encountering,  52 
Trapline,  53,  82 

standard,  26 
Trapped-out  area 

invasion,  82 
Trapping,  76 

comparison    of   results   between   short- 
term  and  long-term  removal,  28 

continuous  removal,  26,  37 

removal,  76 

short-term  removal,  26 


Tribe 

as  a  social  entity,  98 
Trichinella 

increased  invasiveness,  278 
Trichinella  spiralis,  277,  278 
Trichinosis 

effects  of  crowding,  278 
Triiodothyronine,  228,  229,  231 
TSH 

mice,  237 

rabbits,  237 

rats,  237 

rate  of  release,  231 

release,  232,  236 

secretion,  237,  258 
Tuberculosis 

crowding,  effect  on,  279 

murine,  279 
Two-dimensional  field 

use,  19 
Tyrosine,  230 

Ungulates 

captive,  198 

domesticated,  196 
Urine 

metabolites,  207 

Vaginal  orifice 

closure,  291 
Vagus  nerve,  204 
Vasopressin,  214 
Velocity,  164 

biological,  111 

effect  of  interval  between  ^'s  on,  LSI 

effect  of  tranquilizer  on,  172 

effect  on  fat,  174 

effect  on  weights  of  adrenals,  kidney; 
and  heart,  174 

formulae,  111,  112,  113 
a,  112 
aaa,  112 
aap,  112 
afaa,  112 
af'ap,  112 

«mfaa^"\    113 

d,  no 

faa,  112 
f'ap,  112 
i,  112 


380 


Subject  Index 


12,  112 
/i',  112 
N,  110 
Nb,  113 
N^""',  113 
No,  113 
0a,  112 
^a^""),  113 

0a^»',  113 

6>(''),  112 
0f,  112 
0"),  112 

^a'""*,    113 

V,  111 

minimal,  173 

rank,  171 

reduction  in  hierarchj^  of  mice,  148 
Visitation  frequency,  165 
Vitamin  A 

effects  on  behavior,  170 
Vitamin  B 

insufficiency,  222 
Vocalization,  38,  48,  75 

loss  of,  77 
Voles,    (see  also  Microius,  Clethrionomys, 
and  Pitymys),  193,  196,  253,  261,  293, 
317,  318,  322,  323 

adrenalectomized,  218 

adrenal  weight,  285,  286,  301 

adrenocortical-densit}'  relationships,  304 

anestrus,  220 

birth  rates,  292 

competition  for  food,  297 

confined  populations,  272 

copulatory  pressure,  290 

crowding  mortality,  280 

decline  in  birthrate,  291 

decrease  in  reproduction,  286 

depression  of  fertility,  221 

effects  of  high  density,  276 

effect  of  increased  population  density, 
284 

effect  of  overcrowding,  269 

effect  of  population  densities,  275 

eosinophil  counts,  252 

female,  286 

fights,  296 

hypoglycemic  shock,  320,  321 

inhibition  of  growth  and  reproduction, 
316 


intraspecific  strife,  323 

laboratory,  274 

litter  survival,  294 

meadow,  196,  218,  272,  323 

northern  montana,  304 

mortality,  322 

natural  populations,  303 

nests,  294 

ovarian  atrophy,  220 

pine,  196 

population  fluctuation,  312 

red-backed,  196,  311,  312,  314,  318,  323 

relationships  between  population  den- 
sity and  reproduction,  310 

reproductive  function,  314 

responses  to  increased  population  den- 
sity, 281 

responses  to  sociopsychologic  factors, 
269 

self-limited  populations,  299 

social  pressure,  288 

sociopsychologic  factors,  264 

sociopsychologic  interactions,  280 

splenic  hypertrophy,  242,  272,  273,  274 

X-zone,  283 

White-footed  mice  (see  also  Peromyscris), 

273,  323 
White-footed  mice 

population,  305 
Withdrawal,  162 
Withdrawal  state,  180 
Woodchuck  (see  also  Marmota),  193 

relationships   between   population   den- 
sity and  reproduction,  310 
Wound  healing 

delay,  210 

X-zone 

acidophilic  cells,  197 

androgens,  197 

basophilic  nuclei,  197 

Crocidura  russula,  197 

experimental  treatments,  196 

function,  217 

hormones,  196 

pituitary  luteinizing  hormone,  197 

Pitymys  subterraneus,  196,  197 

reactions  to,  196 

shrews,  197 


Subject  Index 


381 


Sorex  araneiis,  197 
voles,  196 

Zona  fasciculata,  217 

carbohydrate-active  corticoid.s,  2C0 
hormones,  205 
hyperplasia,  246 
hypertrophy,  246 
regulation  of  hormones,  213 


Zona  glomerulosa 

aldosterone,  213 

beef  cattle,  200 

hypertrophy,  246 

rats,  200 
Zona  reticularis 

carbohydrate-active  corticoids,  200 

function,  217 

sex  steroids,  217