Skip to main content

Full text of "A text book of physiology"

See other formats


BIOLOGY 


THE  LIBRARY 

OF 

THE  UNIVERSITY 
OF  CALIFORNIA 

IN  MEMORY  OF 

Dr.   and  Mrs. 
Frank  Weymouth 


A    TEXT    BOOK 


OF 


PHYSIOLOGY 


BY 


M.    FOSTER,    M.A.,    M.D.,    LL.D.,    F.E.S., 

PROFESSOR  OF   PHYSIOLOGY   IN   THE   UNIVERSITY   OF   CAMBRIDGE, 
AND  FELLOW  OF  TRINITY  COLLEGE,  CAMBRIDGE. 


WITH    ILLUSTRATIONS. 
SIXTH    EDITION,   LARGELY   REVISED. 

PART    III. 

The  Central  Nervous  System. 


Uonfcon : 
MACMILLAN    AND    CO. 

AND  NEW  YORK. 
1892 

[The  Eight  of  Translation  is  reserved.] 


Cambrttigc : 

PRINTED    BY    C.    J.    CLAY,    M.A.   AND    SONS, 
AT   THE   UNIVERSITY   PRESS. 


First  Edition  1876.     Second  Edition  1877. 
Third  Edition  1879.     Fourth  Edition  1883. 
Reprinted  1884,  1886.     Fifth  Edition  1890. 
Sixth  Edition  1892. 


BIOLOGY 

GIFT 


BIOLOGY 
LIBRARY 


T  AM  of  course  aware  of  the  disadvantages  of  issuing  this 
*-  edition  of  my  Text  Book  in  instalments,  and  very  much 
regret  that  this  part  does  not  complete  the  work.  The  failure 
to  get  the  whole  of  the  remainder  ready  has  been  due  to  lack, 
not  of  will,  but  of  ability  and  opportunity. 

I  take  this  opportunity  of  thanking  my  friend  Dr  Gowers,  for 
the  loan  of  two  woodcuts,  as  well  as  for  much  valuable  advice. 
Throughout  the  whole  of  this  part  I  have  been  largely  assisted  by 
my  colleague  Mr  Langley,  and  by  my  friend  and  former  pupil 
Dr  Sherrington.  The  latter,  besides  helping  me  with  criticisms, 
has  prepared  for  me  most  of  the  figures  after  original  drawings  by 
himself.  What  little  merit  there  may  be  in  this  part  is  largely 
due  to  these  two  gentlemen. 


M.   FOSTER. 


CAMBRIDGE, 

September,  1890. 


861 


CONTENTS   OF   PART   III. 
BOOK  III. 

THE  CENTKAL  NEEVOUS   SYSTEM  AND  ITS  INSTEUMENTS. 

CHAPTER   I. 
THE  SPINAL  CORD. 

SECTION  I. 

ON  SOME  FEATURES  OF  THE  SPINAL  NERVES. 

PAGE 

§  558.    The  spinal  nerves 849 

§  559.    On  efferent  and  afferent  impulses 

§  560.    Efferent  fibres  run  in  the  anterior  root  and  afferent  fibres  in  the 

posterior  root ^52 

§  561.     The  "trophic"  influence  of  the  ganglion  of  the  posterior  root;  the 

degeneration  of  nerve  fibres 853 

SECTION   II. 

THE  STRUCTURE  OF  THE  SPINAL  CORD. 

§  562.     The  general  features  of  the  cord;  grey  and  white  matter          .        .  856 

The  structure  of  the  white  matter ;  neuroglia 859 

564.  The  structure  of  the  grey  matter 861 

565.  The  central  canal,  the  substantia  gelatinosa  centralis,  and  the 

substantia  gelatinosa  of  Eolando .        863 

566.  The  grouping  of  the  nerve  cells.     The  cells  of  the  anterior  and 

posterior  horn,  the  lateral  group,  Clarke's  column,  and  the  lateral 

horn.     The  reticular  formation 865 

567.  The  tracts  of  white  matter.     Median  posterior  column,  external 

posterior  column.  The  evidence  of  the  differentiation  of  the  white 
matter  into  tracts.  Ascending  and  descending  degeneration. 
Descending  tracts :  crossed  and  direct  pyramidal  tracts,  antero- 
lateral  descending  tract.  Ascending  tracts:  cerebellar  tract, 
antero-lateral  ascending  tract,  median  posterior  tract  .  .  .  869 


viii  CONTENTS. 

PAGE 

§568.  The  meaning  of  the  terms  "ascending"  and  "descending"  de- 
generation, and  the  inferences  to  be  drawn  from  them  .  .  .  875 

§  569.  The  connections  of  the  nerve  roots;  of  the  anterior  root;  of  the 

posterior  root,  median,  lateral  and  intermediate  bundles  .  .  876 

§  570.  The  special  features  of  the  several  regions  of  the  spinal  cord.  The 
conus  medullaris,  the  lumbar  and  cervical  swellings.  Variations 
in  the  sectional  area  of  the  white  matter 878 

§  571.    Variations  in  the  sectional  area  of  the  grey  matter  ....        880 

§  572.  The  relative  size,  form  and  features  of  transverse  sections  of  the 

cord  at  different  levels 881 

§  573.  Variations  in  the  disposition  of  nerve  cells  and  groups  of  nerve  cells 

at  different  levels 885 

§  574.    Variations  in  the  several  columns  of  white  matter  at  different  levels.        886 

§  575.  The  course  of  the  crossed  and  of  the  direct  pyramidal  tract  along 

the  length  of  the  cord 888 

§  576.    The  course  of  the  cerebellar  tract  along  the  length  of  the  cord         .        890 

§  577.  The  course  of  the  median  posterior  tract  along  the  length  of  the 

cord 891 

§  578.  The  course  of  the  antero-lateral  ascending  tract  along  the  length  of 

the  cord 895 

§  579.  The  nature  of  the  grey  matter  of  the  cord ;  the  segmental  ground 

work,  the  nerve  cells 895 

§  580.  The  nature  and  relation  to  the  grey  matter  of  the  tracts  of  white 

matter 899 

§  581.    Longitudinal  commissural  tracts,  and  transverse  connections  .        .        900 

SECTION  III. 
THE  REFLEX  ACTIONS  OF  THE  CORD. 

§  582.  The  difficulties  attending  the  experimental  investigation  of  the  cen- 
tral nervous  system;  'shock'  and  other  effects  of  an  operation  .  902 

§  583.  The  differences,  as  regards  reflex  movements,  between  different 

kinds  of  animals 904 

§  584.  The  features  of  a  reflex  act  dependent  on  the  character  of  the 

afferent  impulses 905 

§  585.    The  complex  nature  of  the  central  processes  in  a  reflex  movement    .        906 

§  586.  The  characters  of  a  reflex  movement  dependent  on  the  strength  of 

the  stimulus 906 

§  587.  The  characters  of  a  reflex  movement  dependent  on  the  part  of  the 

body  to  which  the  stimulus  is  applied 907 

§  588.  The  complexity  of  many  reflex  movements ;  their  relation  to  intel- 
ligence   908 

§  589.  Keflex  movements  coordinated  by  afferent  impulses  other  than  the 

exciting  impulses ;  relations  to  consciousness  ....  910 

§  590.  The  characters  of  a  reflex  movement  determined  by  the  intrinsic 

condition  of  the  cord 912 

§  591.     The  reflex  movements  carried  out  by  the  spinal  cord  in  man    .         .         912 

§  592.    Keflex  actions  resulting  in  changes  other  than  movements       .        .        914 

§  593.    The  inhibition  of  reflex  actions .915. 

§  594.     The  time  required  for  reflex  actions .         918 


CONTENTS.  ix 

SECTION  IV. 

THE  AUTOMATIC  ACTIONS  OF  THE  SPINAL  CORD. 

PAGE 

§  595.  Automatic  actions  of  the  spinal  cord  in  the  frog  and  in  the  dog       .  920 

§  596.  Automatic  activity  dependent  on  afferent  impulses  ....  921 

§  597.     Tone  of  skeletal  muscles 922 

§  598.     Tendon  phenomena,  knee  jerk 926 

§  599.    Rigidity  of  muscles  through  spinal  action 927 

CHAPTER   II. 
THE  BRAIN. 

SECTION  I. 

ON  SOME  GENERAL  FEATURES  OF  THE  STRUCTURE  OF  THE  BRAIN. 

§  600.     The  embryonic  brain ;  the  three  primary  cerebral  vesicles         .         .         929 

§  601.  The  transformation  of  these  into  the  bulb  and  fourth  ventricle,  the 
cerebellum  and  pons  varolii,  the  crura  cerebri,  corpora  quadri- 
gemina,  and  third  ventricle 930 

§  602.  The  vesicles  of  the  cerebral  hemispheres,  their  growth  and  trans- 
formation into  the  cerebrum;  the  cerebral  hemispheres,  corpus 
striatum,  corpus  callosum,  fornix,  and  choroid  plexus  .  .  .  930 

§  603.  The  parts  of  the  adult  brain  corresponding  to  the  main  divisions  of 

the  embryonic  brain 935 

§  604.     The  cranial  nerves 936 

SECTION   II. 
THE  BULB. 

§  605.  The  main  changes  by  which  the  cervical  spinal  cord  becomes  trans- 
formed into  the  bulb;  the  pyramids  and  their  decussation,  the 
olivary  bodies,  the  fasciculus  cuneatus  and  fasciculus  gracilis  .  937 

§  606.     The  superior  or  sensory  decussation 942 

§  607.  The  opening  up  of  the  central  canal  of  the  spinal  cord  into  the 

fourth  ventricle  of  the  bulb  ;  the  calamus  scriptorius  .  .  .  943 

§  608.  The  changes  in  the  grey  matter:  the  reticular  formation  and  the 

arcuate  fibres 944 

§  609.  The  olivary  nucleus,  or  inferior  olive,  the  inner  and  outer  accessory 

olivary  nuclei,  the  antero-lateral  nucleus 945 

§  610.  The  gracile  and  cuneate  nuclei;  the  changes  in  the  gelatinous 

substance  of  Eolando 947 

§  611.    The  fibres  of  the  bulb 948 

§  612.  The  relations  of  the  gracile  and  cuneate  nuclei  to  the  inter-olivary 

layer,  to  the  fillet,  and  to  the  restiform  body  ....  949 


xii  CONTENTS. 

SECTION  VII. 
ON  VOLUNTARY  MOVEMENTS. 

PAGE 

§  653.     The  real  distinction  between  voluntary  and  involuntary  movements  .      1034 
§  654.     The  cortical  motor  areas  of  the  dog ;  the  characters  of  the  move- 
ments resulting  from  cortical  stimulation 1035 

§  655.     The  cortical  motor  areas  in  the  monkey 1038 

§  656.     The  cortical  motor  areas  in  the  anthropoid  ape         ....      1043 
§  657.     The  movements  of   cortical  origin  carried  out  by  means  of  the 

pyramidal  tract ;  the  nature  of  the  movements  so  carried  out        .       1044 
§  658.    The  results  of  the  removal  of  a  cortical  area  in  dog  and  in  the 

monkey 1049 

§  659.     The  cortical  motor  areas  in  man ;  the  area  for  speech      .        .        .       1052 
§  660.     The  nature  of  the  action  of  a  motor  area  in  carrying  out  a  voluntary 

movement ;  the  characters  of  aphasia 1056 

The  same  as  illustrated  by  the  area  for  a  limb  in  the  dog;  the 

influence  of  sensory  impulses 1058 

The  relations  of  the  motor  area  to  other  parts  of  the  central  nervous 
system;  the  motor  area  employed  in  movements  usually  called 

involuntary 1061 

§  663.     The  passage  of  volitional  impulses  along  the  spinal  cord  in  animals .       1063 

§  664.     Their  passage  in  man 1065 

§  665.    A  summary  of  the  chief  facts  concerning  the  carrying  out  of 

voluntary  movements 1066 

SECTION   VIII. 

ON  THE  DEVELOPMENT  WITHIN  THE  CENTRAL  NERVOUS  SYSTEM  OF 
VISUAL  AND  OF  SOME  OTHER  SENSATIONS. 

§  666.     Visual  impulses  and  sensations;  visual  fields,  and  binocular  vision .       1070 
§  667.     The  decussation  of  the  optic  nerves  in  the  optic  chiasma          .         .       1073 

§  668.    The  course  of  the  optic  tract 1074 

§  669.  The  endings  of  the  optic  tract  in  the  lateral  corpus  geniculatum,  the 
pulvinar  and  the  anterior  corpus  quadrigeminum ;  the  results  of 

degeneration  and  atrophy  experiments 1075 

§  670.  The  connection  of  the  three  above  bodies  with  the  cerebral  cortex ; 
the  meaning  of  the  terms,  blindness  total  and  complete  or  partial, 
hemianopsia,  amblyopia.  The  difficulties  of  interpretation  attend- 
ing experiments  on  the  vision  of  animals 1076 

§  671.     The  nature  of  the  movements  of  the  eyes  caused  by  stimulation  of 

the  occipital  cortex 1079 

§  672.    The  effects  on  vision  of  removing  parts  of  the  occipital  cortex  in 

monkeys  and  in  dogs;  the  teachings  of  clinical  histories       .        .       1081 
§673.     The  probable  progressive  development  of  visual  sensations;  lower 

and  higher  visual  centres 1083 

§  674.     Sensations  of  smell.     The  structure  of  the  olfactory  bulb  and  tract ; 

the  connections  of  the  tract  with  other  parts  of  the  cerebrum       .       1085 


CONTENTS.  xiii 

PAGE 

675.  The  cortical  area  for  smell 1087 

676.  Sensations  of  taste 1087 

677.  Sensations  of  hearing 1088 


SECTION   IX. 

ON  THE  DEVELOPMENT  OP  CUTANEOUS  AND  SOME  OTHER  SENSATIONS. 

§  678.     Sensations  of  touch,  heat,  cold  and  pain 1090 

§  679.     Theoretical  difficulties  touching  the  cortical  localisation  of  cutaneous 

sensations.     The  effects  on   cutaneous   sensations  of  removing 

regions  of  the  cortex 1091 

§  680.     The  afferent  tracts  from  the  spinal  cord,  their  endings  in  the  brain  .       1094 
§  681.     The  effect  of  sections  of  the  spinal  cord  on  the  transmission  of 

afferent  impulses  influencing  the  vasomotor  centre        .         .         .       1096 
§  682.    Other  experiments  on  animals  as  to  the  effects  of  sections  of  the 

spinal  cord  on  the  transmission  of  sensory  impulses     .         .         .       1099 
§  683.     The  teachings  of  clinical  histories;    different  paths  for  different 

sensory  impulses 1101 

§  684.     General  considerations  on  the  development  of  sensations  along  the 

spinal  cord.     The  cerebellar  tract,  the  median  posterior  tract,  the 

grey  matter  and  internuncial  tracts 1102 

The  terms  'sensory'  and  'motor'  not  an  adequate  description  of  the 

processes  in  the  central  nervous  system 1105 

The  transmission  of  sensations  within  the  brain.     The  relations  of 
the  cerebellum  1106 


SECTION  X. 
ON  SOME  OTHER  ASPECTS  OF  THE  FUNCTIONS  OF  THE  BRAIN. 

§687.     Considerations  touching  the  cerebellum 1109 

§  688.     Considerations  touching  the  corpora  quadrigemina  ....  1112 

§  689.     The  splanchnic  functions  of  the  brain 1114 

§  690.     General  considerations  on  the  processes  taking  place  in  the  cortex. 

The  sources  of  the  energy  of  the  cortex 1115 

SECTION   XL 
ON  THE  TIME  TAKEN  UP  BY  CEREBRAL  OPERATIONS. 

§  691.     The  reaction  period  or  reaction  time 1120 

§  692.  Elementary  analysis  of  psychical  processes,  the  time  taken  up  by 
each.  The  time  required  for  discrimination,  for  the  development 
of  perception,  and  of  the  will ;  the  circumstances  influencing  them  .  1122 


xiv  CONTENTS. 


SECTION  XII. 
THE  LYMPHATIC  ARRANGEMENTS  OF  THE  BRAIN  AND  SPINAL  CORD. 

PAGE 

§  693.     The  membranes  of  the  brain  and  spinal  cord 1125 

§  694.     The  sources  of  the  cerebrospinal  fluid 1126 

§  695.     The  characters  of  the  cerebrospinal  fluid 1128 

The  renewal  of  the  cerebrospinal  fluid.    The  purposes  served  by  the 

fluid  1129 


SECTION   XIII. 

THE  VASCULAR  ARRANGEMENTS  OF  THE  BRAIN  AND  SPINAL  CORD. 

The  distribution  and  characters  of  the  arteries  of  the  brain     .        .      1131 
The  venous  arrangements  of  the  brain ;  the  venous  sinuses      .        .       1133 
§  699.     The  supply  of  blood  to  the  brain  relatively  small.     The  methods  of 

investigating  the  circulation  of  the  brain 1134 

§  700.     The  supply  of  blood  to  the  brain  modified  by  the  respiration  and  by 
changes  in  the  general  arterial  pressure.    The  want  of  clear  proof 
of  special  vasomotor  nerves  for  the  cerebral  arteries      .        .        .      1136 
§  701.     The  flow  of  blood  through  the  brain  nevertheless  influenced  by 

changes  taking  place  in  the  brain  itself 1138 


LIST   OF   FIGURES  IN  PART  III. 


FIG.  PAGE 

96.  A  transverse  dorsoventral  section  of  the  spinal  cord  (human)  at  the 

level  of  the  sixth  thoracic  nerve '  857 

97.  Diagram  to  illustrate  the  nature  of  the  substance  of  Eolando     .        .        864 

98.  Transverse  dorsoventral  section  of  the  spinal  cord  (human)  at  the 

level  of  the  sixth  cervical  nerve 866 

99.  Transverse  dorsoventral  section  of  the  spinal  cord  (human)  at  the 

level  of  the  third  lumbar  nerve 868 

100.  Diagram  to  illustrate  the  general  arrangement  of  the  several  tracts  of 

white  matter  in  the  spinal  cord 872 

101.  Diagram  shewing  the  united  sectional   areas  of  the  spinal  nerves 

proceeding  from  below  upwards 879 

102.  Diagram  shewing  the  variations  in  the  sectional  area  of  the  grey 

matter  of  the  spinal  cord,  along  its  length 880 

103.  Diagram  shewing  the  relative  sectional  areas  of  the  spinal  nerves  as 

they  join  the  spinal  cord 880 

104.  Diagram  illustrating  some  of  the  features  of  the  spinal  cord  at  diffe- 

rent levels 882 

105.  Diagram  shewing  the  variations  in  the  sectional  area  of  the  lateral 

columns  of  the  spinal  cord,  along  its  length 886 

106.  Diagram  shewing  the  variations  in  the  sectional  area  of  the  anterior 

columns  of  the  spinal  cord,  along  its  length 886 

107.  Diagram  shewing  the  variations  in  the  sectional  area  of  the  posterior 

columns  of  the  spinal  cord,  along  its  length 886 

108.  Outlines  of  parts  of  the  brain ;  A  dorsal,  B  lateral,  C  ventral  aspect  .  938 

109.  Transverse  dorsoventral  sections  of  the  bulb  at  different  levels    .        .  940 

110.  Transverse  dorsoventral  section  through  the  bulb  just  behind  the 

pons 948 

111.  Transverse  dorsal  section  through  the  bulb  at  the  widest  part  of  the 

fourth  ventricle 958 

112.  Transverse  dorsoventral  section  through  the  pons  at  the  exit  of  the 

fifth  nerve 961 

113.  Transverse  dorsoventral  section  through  the  fore  part  of  the  pons     .        963 

114.  Transverse  dorsoventral  section  through  the  crus  and  anterior  corpora 

quadrigemina 964 

115.  Diagram  to  illustrate  the  position  of  the  nuclei  of  the  cranial  nerves.        966 

116.  Diagrammatic  outline  of  a  dorsoventral  section  through  the  right 

hemisphere,  at  a  level  just  posterior  to  the  knee  of  the  internal 
capsule 973 


xvi  LIST   OF   FIGURES   IN   PART   III. 

FIG.  PAGE 

117.  Diagrammatic  outline  of  a  dorsoventral  section  through  the  right 

hemisphere  at  a  level  anterior  to  fig.  116 975 

118.  Diagrammatic  outline  of  a  transverse  dorsoventral  section  through 

the  right  hemisphere  through  the  frontal  lobe         ....        976 

119.  Diagrammatic  outline  of  a  sagittal  section  taken  through  the  right 

hemisphere  seen  from  the  mesial  surface 977 

120.  View  of  right  half  of  brain,  as  disclosed  by  a  longitudinal  section  in 

the  median  line  through  the  longitudinal  fissure     ....  979 

121.  Outline  of  horizontal  section  of  brain,  to  shew  the  internal  capsule    .  985 

122.  Outline  of  a  sagittal  section  through  the  hemisphere  ....  986 

123.  Outline  of  a  transverse  dorsoventral  section  of  the  right  half  of  the 

brain 988 

124.  The  areas  of  the  cerebral  convolutions  of  the  dog        ....  1036 

125.  Outline  of  brain  of  monkey  to  shew  the  principal  sulci  and  gyri         .  1040 

126.  Left  hemisphere  of  the  brain  of  monkey  viewed  from  the  left  side  and 

from  above '.  1041 

127.  Mesial  aspect  of  the  left  half  of  the  brain  of  monkey  ....       1042 

128.  Diagram  to  illustrate  the  relative  size  of  the  pyramidal  tract  in  man, 

monkey  and  dog »         .         .         .       1049 

129.  Diagram  of  the  convolutions  and  fissures  on  the  lateral  surface  of  the 

right  cerebral  hemisphere  of  man 1054 

130.  The  same  on  the  mesial  surface 1054 

131.  The  right  lateral  aspect  of  the  cerebrum  of  man  in  outline  to  illustrate 

the  cortical  areas 1055 

132.  Mesial  surface  of  the  right  cerebral  hemisphere  of  man  in  outline  to 

illustrate  the  cortical  areas 1055 

133.  Diagram  to  illustrate  the  nervous  apparatus  of  vision  in  man     .        .      1072 


BOOK    III. 

THE   CENTRAL   NERVOUS   SYSTEM   AND   ITS 
INSTRUMENTS. 


F.  54 


CHAPTER  I. 
THE   SPINAL   CORD. 

SEC.  1.     ON  SOME  FEATURES  OF  THE  SPINAL  NERVES. 

§  558.  WE  have  called  the  muscular  and  nervous  tissues  the 
master  tissues  of  the  body ;  but  a  special  part  of  the  nervous 
system,  that  which  we  know  as  the  central  nervous  system,  the 
brain  and  spinal  cord,  is  supreme  among  the  nervous  tissues  and 
is  master  of  the  skeletal  muscles  as  well  as  of  the  rest  of  the 
body.  We  have  already  (Book  I.  Chap.  III.)  touched  on  some  of 
the  general  features  of  the  nervous  system,  and  have  now  to  study 
in  detail  the  working  of  the  brain  and  spinal  cord.  We  have  to 
inquire  what  we  know  concerning  the  laws  which  regulate  the 
discharge  of  efferent  impulses  from  the  brain  or  from  the  cord, 
and  to  learn  how  that  discharge  is  determined  on  the  one  hand 
by  intrinsic  changes  originating,  apparently,  in  the  substance  of 
the  brain  or  of  the  cord,  and  on  the  other  hand  by  the  nature  and 
amount  of  the  afferent  impulses  which  reach  them  along  afferent 
nerves. 

As  we  shall  see  the  study  of  the  spinal  cord  cannot  be  wholly 
separated  from  that  of  the  brain,  the  two  being  very  closely  related. 
Nevertheless  it  will  be  of  advantage  to  deal  with  the  spinal  cord 
by  itself  as  far  as  we  can.  The  medulla  oblongata  or  spinal 
bulb1  we  shall  consider  as  part  of  the  brain.  But  before  we  speak 

1  The  term  medulla  oblongata  is  not  only  long,  but  presents  difficulties, 
since  the  word  medulla  is  now  rarely  used  to  denote  the  whole  spinal  cord  (medulla 
spinalis)  but  is  generally  used  to  denote  the  peculiar  coat  of  a  nerve  fibre,  the 
white  substance  of  Schwann.  In  using  instead  the  word  bulb  or  if  necessary, 
spinal  bulb  there  is  little  fear  of  confusion  with  any  other  kind  of  bulb.  The 
adjective  is  in  not  uncommon  use,  in  such  phrases  as  'bulbar  paralysis.' 

54—2 


850  SPINAL   NERVES.  [BOOK  HI. 

of  the  spinal  cord  itself,  it  will  be  desirable  to  say  a  few  words 
concerning  the  spinal  nerves,  that  is  to  say  the  nerves  which  issue 
from  the  spinal  cord. 

We  have  already  seen  (§  96)  that  each  of  the  spinal  nerves 
arises  by  two  roots,  an  anterior  root  attached  to  the  ventral  or 
anterior  surface,  and  a  posterior  root  attached  to  the  dorsal  or 
posterior  surface  of  the  cord.  We  have  further  seen  that  the 
latter  bears  a  ganglion,  a  'ganglion  of  the  posterior  root'  or  'spinal 
ganglion,'  and  we  have  (§  97)  studied  the  structure  of  this 
ganglion. 

We  stated  at  the  same  time  that  while  the  trunk  of  a  spinal 
nerve  contained  both  efferent  and  afferent  fibres,  the  efferent 
fibres  were  gathered  up  into  the  anterior  root  and  the  afferent 
fibres  into  the  posterior  root ;  but  we  gave  no  proof  of  this  state- 
ment. 

§  559.  Before  we  proceed  to  do  so,  it  will  be  as  well  to  say  a  few 
words  on  the  terms  '  efferent '  and  '  afferent.'  By  efferent  nerve 
fibres  we  mean  nerve  fibres  which  in  the  body  usually  carry 
impulses  from  the  central  nervous  system  to  peripheral  organs. 
Most  efferent  nerve  fibres  carry  impulses  to  muscles,  striated  or 
plain,  and  the  impulses  passing  along  them  give  rise  to  movements ; 
hence  they  are  frequently  spoken  of  as  'motor'  fibres.  But  all 
efferent  fibres  do  not  end  in  or  carry  impulses  to  muscular  fibres ; 
we  have  seen  for  instance  that  some  efferent  fibres  are  secretory. 
Moreover  all  the  nerve  fibres  going  to  muscular  fibres  do  not  serve 
to  produce  movement;  some  of  them,  as  in  the  case  of  certain  vagus 
fibres  going  to  the  heart,  are  inhibitory  and  may  serve  to  stop 
movement. 

By  '  afferent '  nerve  fibres  we  mean  nerve  fibres  which  in  the 
body  usually  carry  impulses  from  peripheral  organs  to  the  central 
nervous  system.  A  very  common  effect  of  the  arrival  at  the  central 
nervous  system  of  impulses  passing  along  afferent  fibres  is  that 
change  in  consciousness  which  we  call  a  'sensation';  hence  afferent 
fibres  or  impulses  are  often  called  'sensory'  fibres  or  impulses. 
But  as  we  have  already  in  part  seen,  and  as  we  shall  shortly  see 
in  greater  detail,  the  central  nervous  system  may  be  affected  by 
afferent  impulses,  and  that  in  several  ways,  quite  apart  from  the 
development  of  any  such  change  of  consciousness  as  may  be  fairly 
called  a  sensation.  We  shall  see  reason  for  thinking  that  afferent 
impulses  reaching  the  spinal  cord,  and  indeed  other  parts  of  the 
central  nervous  system,  may  modify  reflex  or  automatic  or  other 
activity  without  necessarily  giving  rise  to  a  "  sensation."  Hence  it 
is  advisable  to  reserve  the  terms  '  efferent '  and  '  afferent '  as  more 
general  modes  of  expression  than  '  motor '  or  '  sensory.' 

We  have  seen  in  treating  of  muscle  and  nerve,  that  the  changes 
produced  in  the  muscle  serve  as  our  best  guide  for  determining  the 
changes  taking  place  in  a  motor  nerve ;  when  a  motor  nerve  is 


CHAP,  i.]  THE   SPINAL   CORD.  851 

separated  from  its  muscle  (§  72)  the  only  change  which  we  can 
appreciate  in  it  is  an  electrical  change.  Similarly  in  the  case  of 
an  afferent  nerve,  the  central  system  is  our  chief  teacher ;  in  a 
bundle  of  afferent  fibres  isolated  from  the  central  nervous  system, 
in  a  posterior  root  of  a  spinal  nerve  for  instance,  the  only  change 
which  we  can  appreciate  is  an  electrical  change.  To  learn  the 
characters  of  afferent  impulses  we  must  employ  the  central  nervous 
system.  But  in  this  we  meet  with  difficulties.  In  studying  the 
phenomena  of  motor  nerves  we  are  greatly  assisted  by  two  facts. 
First,  the  muscular  contraction  by  which  we  judge  of  what  is  going 
on  in  the  nerve  is  a  comparatively  simple  thing,  one  contraction 
differing  from  another  only  by  such  features  as  extent  or  amount, 
duration,  frequency  of  repetition  and  the  like,  and  all  such 
differences  are  capable  of  exact  measurement.  Secondly,  when 
we  apply  a  stimulus  directly  to  the  nerve  itself,  the  effects  differ 
in  degree  only  from  those  which  result  when  the  nerve  is  set 
in  action  by  natural  stimuli,  such  as  the  will.  When  we  come,  on 
the  other  hand,  to  investigate  the  phenomena  of  afferent  nerves, 
our  labours  are  for  the  time  rendered  heavier,  but  in  the  end 
more  fruitful,  by  the  following  circumstances : — First,  when  we 
judge  of  what  is  going  on  in  an  afferent  nerve  by  the  effects 
which  stimulation  of  the  nerve  produces  in  some  central  nervous 
organ,  in  the  way  of  exciting  or  modifying  reflex  action,  or 
modifying  automatic  action,  or  affecting  consciousness,  we  are 
met  on  the  very  threshold  of  every  enquiry  by  the  difficulty  of 
clearly  distinguishing  the  events  which  belong  exclusively  to  the 
afferent  nerve  from  those  which  belong  to  the  central  organ. 
Secondly,  the  effects  of  applying  a  stimulus  to  the  peripheral  end- 
organ  of  an  afferent  nerve  are  very  different  from  those  of  applying 
the  same  stimulus  directly  to  the  nerve-trunk.  This  may  be 
shewn  by  the  simple  experience  of  comparing  the  sensation  caused 
by  bringing  any  sharp  body  into  contact  with  a  nerve  laid  bare 
in  a  wound  with  that  caused  by  contact  of  an  intact  skin  with  the 
same  body.  These  and  like  differences  reveal  to  us  a  complexity 
of  impulses,  of  which  the  phenomena  of  motor  nerves  gave  us 
hardly  a  hint. 

We  shall  further  see  in  detail  later  on  that  our  consciousness 
may  be  affected  in  many  different  ways  by  afferent  impulses; 
we  must  distinguish  not  only  sensory  from  other  afferent  impulses, 
but  also  different  kinds  of  sensory  impulses  from  each  other. 
Certain  afferent  nerves  are  spoken  of  as  nerves  of  special  sense, 
and  the  nature  of  the  afferent  impulses  passing  along  these  special 
nerves  together  with  the  modifications  of  consciousness  caused  by 
arrival  of  these  impulses  at  the  central  nervous  system  constitute 
by  themselves  a  complex  and  difficult  branch  of  study.  In  some 
of  the  problems  connected  with  the  central  nervous  system  we 
shall  have  to  appeal  to  the  results  of  a  study  of  these  special 


852  SPINAL   NERVES.  [BOOK  in. 

senses;  but,  on  the  other  hand,  a  knowledge  of  the  central  nervous 
system  is  necessary  to  a  proper  understanding  of  the  special 
senses;  and  on  the  whole  it  will  be  more  convenient  to  study 
the  former  before  the  latter. 

We  may,  however,  digress  here  to  remark  that  the  question 
whether  an  afferent  impulse  differs  in  itself  from  an  efferent 
impulse  is  one  of  great  difficulty.  It  is  true  that  the  electrical 
changes,  which  alone  as  we  have  said  we  can  appreciate  in  an 
isolated  piece  of  nerve,  appear  to  be  the  same  in  both  kinds  of 
fibres;  in  each  the  electrical  change  is  propagated  in  both  directions 
and  possesses  the  same  features.  But  it  would  be  hazardous  to 
insist  too  much  on  this.  Moreover,  we  must  remember  that  what 
we  call  a  nervous  impulse,  especially  one  provoked  by  artificial 
stimulation,  constitutes  a  gross  change  in  the  nerve  fibre,  and 
that  changes  of  a  finer,  more  delicate  nature,  such  as  cannot  be 
shewn  by  the  coarse  methods  used  to  detect  a  '  nervous  impulse,' 
may  take  place  in,  and  be  propagated  along,  a  nerve  fibre.  We 
shall  have  occasion  immediately  to  point  out  that  the  condition  of 
an  afferent  nerve  fibre  along  its  whole  length  is  dependent  on  a 
nerve  cell  in  the  ganglion  of  the  posterior  root ;  the  fibre  when 
cut  off  from  the  nerve  cell  degenerates  and  dies.  This  means 
that  in  the  intact  fibre  certain  influences  are  propagated  along 
the  fibre  from  the  cell  in  the  ganglion  to  the  peripheral  endings 
of  the  fibre,  that  is  to  say  in  a  direction  the  opposite  of  that  taken 
by  the  ordinary  afferent  nervous  impulses ;  and  it  may  be  that  in 
like  manner  in  efferent  fibres  some  influences  are  propagated 
centripetally  from  the  peripheral  endings  to  the  central  nervous 
system.  Our  knowledge  of  these  influences  is  extremely  limited : 
but  it  is  important  to  bear  in  mind  the  possibility  of  their 
occurrence.  And  we  had  this  in  view,  when  above,  in  speaking  of 
efferent  and  afferent  fibres,  we  used  the  phrase  "usually  carry 
impulses." 

§  560.  The  proof  that  the  afferent  and  efferent  fibres  which 
are  both  present  in  the  trunk  of  a  spinal  nerve  are  parted  at  the 
roots,  the  efferent  fibres  running  exclusively  in  the  ventral  or 
anterior  root  and  the  afferent  fibres  exclusively  in  the  dorsal  or 
posterior  root,  is  as  follows. 

When  the  anterior  root  is  divided,  the  muscles  supplied  by  the 
nerve  cease  to  be  thrown  into  contractions  either  by  the  will,  or  by 
reflex  action,  while  the  structures  to  which  the  nerve  is  distributed 
retain  their  sensibility.  During  the  section  of  the  root,  or  when 
the  proximal  stump,  that  connected  with  the  spinal  cord,  is  stimu- 
lated, no  sensory  effects  are  produced.  When  the  distal  stump  is 
stimulated,  the  muscles  supplied  by  the  nerve  are  thrown  into 
contractions.  When  the  posterior  root  is  divided,  the  muscles 
supplied  by  the  nerve  continue  to  be  thrown  into  action  by  an 
exercise  of  the  will  or  as  part  of  a  reflex  action,  but  the  structures 


CHAP,  i.]  THE   SPINAL   CORD.  853 

to  which  the  nerve  is  distributed  lose  the  sensibility  which  they 
previously  possessed.  During  the  section  of  the  root,  and  when  the 
proximal  stump  is  stimulated,  sensory  effects  are  produced.  When 
the  distal  stump  is  stimulated  no  movements  are  called  forth. 
These  facts  demonstrate  that  sensory  impulses  pass  exclusively  by 
the  posterior  root  from  the  peripheral  to  the  central  organs,  and 
that  motor  impulses  pass  exclusively  by  the  anterior  root  from  the 
central  to  the  peripheral  organs ;  and  as  far  as  our  knowledge 
goes  the  same  holds  good  not  only  for  sensory  and  motor  but  also 
for  afferent  and  efferent  impulses. 

An  exception  must  be  made  to  the  above  general  statement,  on 
account  of  the  so-called  "  recurrent  sensibility  "  which  is  witnessed 
in  conscious  mammals,  under  certain  circumstances.  It  some- 
times happens  that  when  the  distal  stump  of  the  divided  anterior 
root  is  stimulated,  signs  of  pain  are  witnessed.  These  are  not 
caused  by  the  concurrent  muscular  contractions  or  cramp  which  the 
stimulation  occasions,  for  they  persist  after  the  whole  trunk  of  the 
nerve  has  been  divided  some  little  way  below  the  union  of  the  roots 
above  the  origins  of  the  muscular  branches,  so  that  no  contractions 
take  place.  They  disappear  when  the  posterior  root  is  subse- 
quently divided,  and  they  are  not  seen  if  the  mixed  nerve  trunk 
be  divided  close  to  the  union  of  the  roots.  The  phenomena  are 
probably  due  to  the  fact,  that  bundles  of  sensory  fibres  of  the 
posterior  root  after  running  a  short  distance  down  the  mixed 
trunk  turn  back  and  run  upwards  in  the  anterior  root,  (being 
distributed  probably  to  the  pia  mater)  and  by  this  recurrent  course 
give  rise  to  the  recurrent  sensibility. 

§  561.  Concerning  the  ganglion  on  the  posterior  root,  we  may 
say  definitely  that  we  have  no  evidence  that  it  can  act  as  a  centre 
of  reflex  action;  nor  have  we  any  evidence  that  it  can  spontaneously 
give  origin  to  efferent  impulses  and  thus  act  as  an  automatic 
centre,  as  can  the  central  nervous  system  itself.  The  bodies  of 
the  nerve-cells  behave  somewhat  differently  from  the  axis-cylinders 
at  some  distance  from  the  cells,  though,  as  we  have  seen,  these  are 
in  reality  processes  of  the  nerve  cells ;  thus  the  nerve  cells  in  the 
ganglion  appear  to  be  more  sensitive  to  certain  poisons  than  are 
the  nerve  fibres  of  the  nerve  trunk.  But  beyond  this,  our  know- 
ledge concerning  the  function  of  the  ganglion  is  almost  limited  to 
the  fact  that  it  is  in  some  way  intimately  connected  with  the 
nutrition  of  the  nerve.  As  we  have  already  (§  83)  said,  when  a 
mixed  nerve  trunk  is  divided  the  peripheral  portion  degenerates 
from  the  point  of  section  downwards  towards  the  periphery.  The 
central  portion  does  not  so  degenerate,  and  if  the  length  of  nerve 
removed  be  not  too  great,  the  central  portion  may  grow  downwards 
along  the  course  of  the  degenerating  peripheral  portion,  and  thus 
regenerate  the  nerve.  This  degeneration  is  observed  when  the 
mixed  trunk  is  divided  in  any  part  of  its  course  from  the  periphery 


854  SPINAL   NERVES.  [BOOK  m 

to  close  up  to  the  ganglion.  When  the  posterior  root  is  divided 
between  the  ganglion  and  the  spinal  cord,  the  portion  attached 
to  the  spinal  cord  degenerates,  but  that  attached  to  the  ganglion 
remains  intact.  When  the  anterior  root  is  divided,  the  proximal 
portion  in  connection  with  the  spinal  cord  remains  intact,  but 
the  distal  portion  between  the  section  and  the  junction  with 
the  other  root  degenerates ;  and  in  the  mixed  nerve-trunk 
many  degenerated  fibres  are  seen,  which,  if  they  be  carefully 
traced  out,  are  found  to  be  motor  (efferent)  fibres.  If  the 
posterior  root  be  divided  carefully  between  the  ganglion  and  the 
junction  with  the  anterior  root,  the  small  portion  of  the  posterior 
root  left  attached  to  the  peripheral  side  of  the  ganglion  above 
the  section  remains  intact,  as  does  also  the  rest  of  the  root 
from  the  ganglion  to  the  spinal  cord,  but  in  the  mixed  nerve- 
trunk  are  seen  numerous  degenerated  fibres,  which  when  examined 
are  found  to  have  the  distribution  of  sensory  (afferent)  fibres. 
Lastly,  if  the  posterior  ganglion  be  excised,  the  whole  posterior 
root  degenerates,  as  do  also  the  sensory  (afferent)  fibres  of  the 
mixed  nerve  trunk.  Putting  all  these  facts  together,  it  would 
seem  that  the  growth  of  the  efferent  and  afferent  fibres  takes 
place  in  opposite  directions,  and  starts  from  different  nutritive 
or  '  trophic '  centres.  The  afferent  fibres  grow  away  from  the 
ganglion  either  towards  the  periphery,  or  towards  the  spinal  cord. 
The  efferent  fibres  grow  outwards  from  the  spinal  cord  towards 
the  periphery.  This  difference  in  their  mode  of  nutrition  is 
frequently  of  great  help  in  investigating  the  relative  distribution 
of  efferent  and  afferent  fibres.  When  a  posterior  root  is  cut 
beyond  the  ganglion,  or  the  ganglion  excised,  all  the  afferent 
nerves  degenerate,  and  in  the  mixed  nerve  branches  these  afferent 
fibres,  by  their  altered  condition,  can  readily  be  traced.  Con- 
versely, when  the  anterior  roots  are  cut,  the  efferent  fibres  alone 
degenerate,  and  can  be  similarly  recognized  in  a  mixed  nerve  tract. 
When  the  anterior  root  is  divided  some  few  fibres  in  it  do  not, 
like  the  rest,  degenerate,  and  when  the  posterior  root  is  divided, 
a  few  fibres  in  the  anterior  root  are  seen  to  degenerate  like  those 
of  the  posterior  root ;  these  appear  to  be  the  fibres  which  give 
to  the  anterior  root  its  "recurrent  sensibility."  In  the  case  of 
certain  spinal  nerves  at  all  events,  it  has  also  been  ascertained 
that  when  the  posterior  root  is  divided,  while  most  of  the  fibres 
in  the  part  of  the  root  thus  cut  off  from  the  ganglion  but  left 
attached  to  the  cord  degenerate,  some  few  do  not.  These  few 
appear  to  have  their  trophic  centre  not  in  the  ganglion,  but 
in  some  part  of  the  spinal  cord  itself;  we  shall  refer  to  these 
later  on. 

This  method  of  distinguishing  nerve  fibres  by  the  features 
of  their  degeneration,  called  the  "degeneration  method,"  or 
sometimes  from  the  name  of  the  physiologist  who  introduced 


CHAP,  i.]  THE   SPINAL   CORD.  855 

it,  the  "  Wallerian  method,"  has  proved  of  great  utility.  Thus 
in  the  vagus  nerve  which  is  composed  not  only  of  fibres  which 
spring  from  the  real  vagus  root  but  also  of  fibres  proceeding  from 
the  spinal  accessory  roots,  the  two  may  be  distinguished  by 
section  of  the  vagus  and  spinal  accessory  roots  respectively. 
We  shall  presently  see  that  this  method  may  be  applied  to 
the  differentiation  of  tracts  of  fibres  in  the  brain  and  spinal 
cord. 


SEC.    2.     THE   STRUCTURE   OF  THE   SPINAL   CORD. 


§  562.  Lying  within  the  vertebral  canal  the  spinal  cord  is 
protected  by  its  'membranes,'  the  dura  mater,  the  arachnoid 
membrane  and  the  pia  mater.  The  consideration  of  the  arrange- 
ment of  these  membranes  and  of  the  structure  of  the  dura  mater 
and  arachnoid  we  will  leave  until  we  come  to  speak  of  the  vascular 
and  lymphatic  supplies  of  the  central  nervous  system ;  the  histo- 
logy of  the  pia  mater  may  more  fitly  come  with  that  of  the  spinal 
cord  itself. 

Along  its  whole  length  from  its  junction  with  the  bulb  to 
its  termination  in  the  filum  terminate  the  spinal  cord,  while 
possessing  certain  general  features,  is  continually  changing  as  to 
special  features.  It  will  be  convenient  to  study  first  the  general 
structure  of  some  particular  part,  for  instance  the  middle  of  the 
thoracic  (dorsal)1  region,  and  afterwards  to  point  out  the  special 
features  which  obtain  in  the  several  regions. 

A  transverse  vertical  section  of  either  a  fresh  or  a  hardened  and 
prepared  spinal  cord  at  the  thoracic  region  possesses  an  outline 
which  is  roughly  speaking  circular.  In  the  middle  of  the  anterior 
or  ventral  surface  is  a  vertical  fissure,  the  ventral  or  anterior  fissure 
(Fig.  96,  A.  F.)  running  some  way  across  the  thickness  of  the  cord 
from  the  ventral  towards  the  dorsal  surface.  Opposite  to  it  on 
the  posterior  or  dorsal  surface  is  a  corresponding,  deeper  but 
narrower,  dorsal  or  posterior  fissure  (Fig.  96,  P.  F.)  which, 
however,  as  we  shall  see,  differs  materially  in  nature  from  the 

1  It  is  very  desirable  to  use  the  terms  '  dorsal '  and  '  ventral '  for  the  parts  of  the 
cerebro-spinal  axis  which  lie  respectively  near  the  dorsal  or  back  part,  and  the 
ventral  or  belly  part  of  the  body,  instead  of  the  terms  posterior  and  anterior;  but 
if  this  is  done,  the  use  of  the  word  dorsal  to  denote  the  region  of  the  cord  between 
the  lumbar  and  cervical  regions  is  apt  to  lead  to  confusion ;  hence  the  introduction 
of  the  word  thoracic.  If  this  use  of  dorsal  and  ventral  be  adhered  to,  before  and 
behind,  above  and  below,  may  conveniently  be  used  to  denote  nearer  the  head  and 
nearer  the  tail  (or  coccyx)  respectively ;  anterior  and  posterior  may  also  be  used  in 
the  same  sense  except  in  the  case  of  anterior  and  posterior  fissure  and  horn,  which 
terms  seem  too  much  honoured  by  time  to  be  thrown  aside. 


CHAP,  i.] 


THE   SPINAL   CORD. 


857 


P.r. 


FIG.  96.       A  TBANSVEKSB  DOKSO VENTRAL  SECTION  OF  THE   SPINAL   COED   (HUMAN) 

AT  THE  LEVEL  or  THE  SIXTH  THORACIC  (DORSAL)  NERVE.     (Sherrington)1. 
Magnified  15  times.     One  lateral  half  only  is  shewn.     The  large  conspicuous 

nerve-cells  (drawn  from  actual  specimens)  are  shaded  black  to  render  their  relative 

size,  shape  and  position  more  obvious ;  the  outline  of  the  grey  matter  has  been 

made  thick  and  dark  in  order  to  render  it  'conspicuous. 

A.F.  anterior  fissure.  P.F.  posterior  fissure,  c.c.  central  canal,  c.g.s.  central 
gelatinous  substance.  A.r.  anterior  root,  P.r.  lateral  (or  intermediate)  bundle, 
P.r'.  median  bundle  of  posterior  root  of  spinal  nerve,  p',  p"  fibres  of  posterior 
root  passing  p',  indirectly  through  the  substance  of  Rolando,  p",  directly  into 
grey  matter,  a.g.c.  anterior  grey  commissure,  p-g.c.  posterior  grey  com- 
missure, a.c.  anterior  white  commissure,  ant.  col.  anterior  column,  lat.  col. 
lateral  column,  post.  col.  posterior  column,  s.g.  the  substance  of  Rolando. 
s.  septum  marking  out  the  external  posterior  column  or  column  of  Burdach, 
e.p.,  from  the  median  posterior  column  or  column  of  Goll,  m.p. 

1.  cells  of  the  anterior  horn.  3.  posterior  vesicular  column  or  vesicular  cylinder, 
or  column  of  Clarke;  the  area  of  the  cylinder  is  defined  by  a  dotted  line.  4.  cells 
of  the  intermedio-lateral  tract  or  lateral  horn.  6.  cells  of  the  posterior  horn. 
7.  cells  of  the  anterior  cervix,  y.  a  tract  of  fibres  passing  from  the  vesicular 
cylinder  to  the  lateral  column. 

1  For  this  and  many  succeeding  figures  I  am  deeply  indebted  to  my  friend  and 
former  pupil  Dr  Sherrington  who  has  kindly  prepared  the  figures  for  me  from  his 
original  drawings. 


858  STRUCTURE   OF   SPINAL   CORD.          [BOOK  m. 

anterior  fissure,  and  ought  to  be  called  a  septum  rather  than  a 
fissure.  Between  the  two  fissures  the  substance  of  the  cord  is 
reduced  to  a  narrow  isthmus  uniting  the  two  lateral  halves,  which 
in  a  normal  cord  are  like  each  other  in  every  respect.  In  the 
middle  of  the  isthmus  lies  the  section  of  a  small  canal,  the  central 
canal  (Fig.  96,  c,  c.),  which  is  all  that  remains  of  the  relatively 
wide  neural  canal  of  the  embryo. 

Each  lateral  half  consists  of  an  outer  zone  of  white  matter 
surrounding,  except  at  the  isthmus,  an  inner  more  or  less 
crescentic,  or  comma  shaped  mass  of  grey  matter.  The  convexity 
of  each  crescent  is  turned  towards  the  median  line  of  the  cord,  the 
two  crescents  being  placed  back  and  back  and  joined  together 
by  the  isthmus  just  spoken  of.  The  somewhat  broader  anterior 
extremity  of  the  crescent,  or  head  of  the  comma,  is  called  the 
anterior  cornu  or  horn ;  and  the  narrower  posterior  extremity  of 
the  crescent,  or  tail  of  the  comma,  is  called  the  posterior  cornu  or 
horn.  The  part  by  which  each  horn  is  joined  on  to  the  middle 
part  of  the  crescent  is  called  the  cervix,  anterior  and  posterior 
respectively.  The  isthmus  joining  the  backs  of  the  two  crescents, 
like  the  crescents  themselves,  consists,  for  the  most  part,  of  grey 
matter,  the  band  running  posterior  or  dorsal  to  the  central  canal 
being  called  the  posterior  grey  commissure  (Fig.  96,  p.  g.  c),  and 
the  band  running  anterior  or  ventral  to  the  canal  being  called  the 
anterior  grey  commissure  (Fig.  96,  a.  g.  c.).  The  posterior  fissure 
touches  the  posterior  grey  commissure, -but  the  anterior  grey 
commissure  is  separated  from  the  bottom  of  the  anterior  fissure 
by  a  band  of  white  matter,  called  the  anterior  white  commissure 
or,  more  simply,  the  white  commissure  or  sometimes  the  anterior 
commissure  (Fig.  96,  a.  c.). 

If  the  section  be  taken  at  the  level  of  the  origin  of  a  pair  of 
spinal  nerves,  it  will  be  seen  that  the  anterior  or  ventral  root, 
piercing  the  white  matter  opposite  the  head  of  the  comma  in 
several  distinct  bundles  (Fig.  96,  A.r.\  plunges  into  the  anterior 
cornu,  while  the  posterior  or  dorsal  root  (Fig.  96,  P.r.,  P.r'.),  having 
the  appearance  of  a  single  undivided  bundle,  passes,  in  part  at 
least,  into  the  posterior  horn.  Both  roots  are  dispersed  length- 
ways along  the  cord,  the  hinder  roots  of  one  nerve  being  close  to 
the  foremost  roots  of  the  nerve  below,  but  it  is  only  the  anterior- 
roots  which  are  dispersed  sideways.  The  compact  bundle  of  the 
posterior  root  divides,  with  tolerable  sharpness,  the  white  matter 
in  each  lateral  half  of  the  cord  into  a  posterior  portion  lying 
between  the  posterior  fissure  and  the  posterior  root  (Fig.  96,  post, 
col),  which  portion  since,  as  we  shall  see,  it  runs  in  the  form 
of  a  column  along  the  length  of  the  cord,  is  called  the  posterior 
column,  arid  into  a  portion  lying  to  the  outside  of  the  posterior 
root  between  it  and  the  anterior  fissure,  called  the  antero- 
lateral  column.  This  latter  may  be  considered  as  further  divided, 
by  the  entrance  of  the  anterior  roots  into  a  lateral  column  (Fig.  96, 


CHAP,  i.]  THE   SPINAL   CORD.  859 

lat.  col)  between  the  posterior  root  and  the  most  external  bundle 
of  the  anterior  root,  and  into  an  anterior  column  (Fig.  96,  ant. 
col.)  between  the  anterior  fissure  and  the  most  external  bundle 
of  the  anterior  root.  The  part  traversed  by  the  bundles  of  the 
anterior  root,  as  they  make  for  the  anterior  horn,  accordingly 
belongs  to  the  anterior  column ;  but  some  writers  speak  of  the 
anterior  column  as  lying  between  the  anterior  fissure  and  the 
nearest  bundle  of  the  anterior  root,  thus  making  the  region  of  the 
anterior  root  belong  to  neither  anterior  nor  lateral  column.  And 
indeed  the  distinction  between  the  anterior  and  the  lateral  column 
is  to  a  great  extent  an  artificial  distinction. 

§  563.  The  'white  matter'  consists  exclusively  of  medullated 
fibres  supported  partly  by  connective  tissue  and  partly  by  a  peculiar 
tissue  known  as  neuroglia,  of  which  we  shall  presently  speak.  The 
fibres  are  of  various  sizes,  but  many  of  them  are  large,  and  in  all 
of  them  the  medulla  is  conspicuous.  They  run  for  the  most  part 
longitudinally,  so  that  in  transverse  sections  of  the  cord  nearly  the 
whole  of  the  white  matter  appears  under  the  microscope  to  be 
composed  of  minute  circles,  the  transverse  sections  of  the  lon- 
gitudinally disposed  fibres,  imbedded  in  the  supporting  structures. 
The  '  grey  matter '  also  contains  medullated  fibres,  but  these  are 
for  the  most  part  exceedingly  fine  fibres  possessing  a  medulla 
which  appears  to  differ  from  that  of  an  ordinary  nerve  fibre,  since 
it  does  not  stain  readily  with  osmic  acid,  but  is  rendered  visible  by 
special  modes  of  preparation  such  as  that  known  as  Weigert's. 
Hence  these  fine  fibres  are  not  apparent  in  ordinary  carmine  or 
other  specimens,  and  indeed  their  presence  was  for  a  long  time 
overlooked.  Besides  these  fine  medullated  fibres,  if  we  may  call 
them  such,  the  grey  matter  contains,  what  the  white  matter  does 
not,  nerve-cells  with  branching  processes,  naked  axis- cylinders,  and 
delicate  filaments  arising  from  the  division  of  axis-cylinders  or 
from  the  branching  of  nerve-ceHs,  all  these  various  structures 
being  imbedded  in  neuroglia.  Owing  to  the  relative  abundance 
of  the  white  refractive  medulla,  the  white  matter  possesses  in 
fresh  specimens  a  characteristic  opaque  white  colour;  hence  the 
name.  The  grey  matter  from  the  relative  scantiness  of  medulla 
has  no  such  opaque  whiteness,  is  much  more  translucent,  and 
in  fresh  specimens  has  a  grey  or  rather  pinky  grey  colour,  the 
reddish  tint  being  due  to  the  presence  partly  of  pigment  and 
partly  of  blood,  for  the  blood  vessels  are  much  more  abundant  in 
the  grey  matter  than  in  the  white. 

The  pia  mater  which  closely  invests  the  cord  all  round  consists 
of  connective  tissue,  fairly  rich  in  elastic  elements  and  abun- 
dantly supplied  with  blood  vessels ;  it  is  indeed  essentially  a 
vascular  membrane  and  furnishes  the  nervous  elements  of  the 
cord  with  their  chief  supply  of  blood.  It  sends  in  at  intervals 
partitions  or  septa  of  the  same  nature  as  itself  radiating  towards 
the  central  grey  matter.  The  narrow  posterior  fissure  is  com- 


860  STRUCTURE   OF   SPINAL   CORD.          [BOOK  in. 

pletely  filled  up  by  a  large  septum  of  this  kind,  indeed  as  we 
have  said  is  in  reality  not  a  fissure  but  a  large  septum ;  but  the 
anterior  fissure  is  too  wide  for  such  an  arrangement ;  the  whole 
membrane  dips  down  into  this  fissure,  following  the  surface  of  the 
cord  and  being  reflected  at  the  bottom.  From  these  primary 
septa,  secondar}^  finer  septa  still  composed  of  ordinary  fibrillated 
connective  tissue,  carrying  blood  vessels,  branch  off ;  but  these  are 
soon  merged  into  the  peculiar  supporting  tissue  called,  as  we  have 
said,  neuroglia.  This  consists  in  the  first  place  of  small  branching 
cells,  lying  in  various  planes.  The  branching  is  excessive,  so  that 
the  body  of  the  cell  is  reduced  to  very  small  dimensions,  indeed 
at  times  almost  obliterated,  the  nucleus  disappearing  while  the 
numerous  branches  are  continued  as  long  fine  filaments  or  fibres 
pursuing  a  devious  but  for  the  most  part  a  longitudinal  course. 
In  the  second  place  these  cells  and  fibres  or  filaments  are  im- 
bedded in  a  homogeneous  ground  substance.  Relatively  to  the 
fibres  and  ground  substance  the  bodies  of  the  cells  (which  are 
called  Deiter's  cells),  especially  bodies  such  as  bear  obvious  nuclei, 
are  very  scanty;  hence  in  sections,  especially  in  transverse 
sections,  of  the  cord  the  neuroglia  has  often  a  dotted  or  punctated 
appearance,  the  dots  being  the  transverse  sections  of  the  fine  lon- 
gitudinally disposed  fibres  imbedded  in  the  ground  substance. 
Examined  chemically  the  neuroglia  is  found  to  be  composed  not 
like  connective  tissue  of  gelatine,  but  of  a  substance  which  appears 
to  be  closely  allied  to  keratin,  the  chief  constituent  of  horny 
epidermis,  hairs  and  the  like,  §  435,  and  which  has  therefore  been 
called  neurokeratin,  (see  also  §  68).  And  indeed  this  neuroglia, 
though  like  connective  tissue  a  supporting  structure,  is  not,  like 
connective  tissue,  of  mesoblastic,  but  of  epiblastic  origin.  The 
walls  of  the  neural  canal  of  the  embryo  which  are  transformed 
into  the  spinal  cord  of  the  adult  consist  at  first  of  epithelial, 
epiblastic  cells;  and  while  some  of  these  cells  become  nervous 
elements,  others  become  neuroglia.  The  epithelial  cells  which  are 
destined  to  form  neuroglia  become  exceedingly  branched,  while 
their  originally  protoplasmic  cell-substance  becomes  transformed  to 
a  large  extent  into  neurokeratin. 

The  neuroglia  fills  up  the  spaces  between  the  radiating  larger 
septal  prolongations  of  the  pia  mater  and  the  finer  branched  septa 
which  starting  from  the  larger  ones  carry  minute  blood  vessels  into 
the  interior  of  the  white  matter.  In  these  spaces  it  is  so  arranged 
as  to  form  delicate  tubular  canals,  of  very  variable  size,  running 
for  the  most  part  in  a  longitudinal  direction.  Each  of  these  tubular 
canals  is  occupied  by  and  wholly  filled  up  with  a  medullated 
nerve  fibre  of  corresponding  size.  A  medullated  nerve  fibre  of  the 
white  matter  of  the  spinal  cord  resembles  a  medullated  nerve 
fibre  of  a  nerve  (§  68)  in  being  composed  of  an  axis-cylinder  and  a 
medulla;  but  it  possesses  no  primitive  sheath  or  neurilemma. 
This  is  absent  and  indeed  is  not  wanted;  the  tubular  sheath  of 


il 


CHAP.  L]  THE   SPINAL   CORD.  861 

neuroglia  affords  in  the  spinal  cord  (and  as  we  shall  see  in  the 
central  nervous  system  generally)  the  support  which  in  a  nerve 
is  afforded  by  the  neurilemma.  Nodes  are,  according  to  most 
authors,  absent,  but  some  say  they  are  present. 

The  white  matter  of  the  cord  consists  then  of  a  more  or  less 
solid  mass  of  neuroglia,  having  the  structure  just  described,  which 
is  permeated  by  minute  canals,  some  exceedingly  fine  and  carrying 
very  fine  2//,  fibres,  others  larger  and  carrying  fibres  up  to  the  size 
of  15yL6.  This  mass  is  further  broken  up.  into  areas  by  the  smaller 
and  larger  vascular  connective-tissue  septa  with  the  edges  and 
endings  of  which  the  neuroglia  is  continuous.  Most  of  the  nerve- 
fibres,  as  we  have  said,  run  longitudinally  and  in  a  transverse 
section  of  the  cord  are  cut  transversely ;  but  as  we  shall  see 
fibres  are  continually  passing  into  and  out  of  the  white  matter, 
and  in  so  doing  take  a  more  or  less  transverse  course ;  these 
however  are  few  compared  with  those  which  run  in  a  longitudinal 
direction.  On  the  outside  of  the  cord  below  the  pia  mater  the 
neuroglia  is  developed  into  a  layer  of  some  thickness  from  which 
nerve  fibres  are  absent;  this  is  often  spoken  of  as  an  inner  layer  of 
the  pia  mater;  but  being  neuroglia  and  not  connective  tissue  is  of 
a  different  nature  from  the  pia  mater  proper.  A  layer  of  this 
superficial  neuroglia  also  accompanies  the  larger  septa,  and  a 
considerable  quantity  is  present  in  the  large  septum  called  the 
posterior  fissure. 

The  pia  mater  carries  not  only  blood  vessels  but  also  lymphatics; 
of  these  however  we  shall  speak  when  we  come  to  deal  with  the 
vascular  arrangements  of  the  whole  of  the  central  nervous  system. 

§  564.  In  the  grey  matter  we  may  distinguish  the  larger, 
more  conspicuous  nerve-cells  and  the  rest  of  the  grey  matter  in 
which  these  cells  lie.  We  have  already  (§  99)  described  the 
general  features  of  these  larger  nerve-cells,  and  shall  have  pre- 
sently to  speak  of  their  special  characters  and  grouping.  Mean- 
while the  most  important  point  to  remember  about  them  besides 
the  fact  that  they  vary  largely  in  form  and  size  is  that  while  one 
process  may  or  does  become  an  axis-cylinder  of  a  nerve  fibre,  the 
others  rapidly  branch,  and  breaking  up  into  fine  nerve  filaments 
are  lost  to  view  in  the  rest  of  the  grey  matter. 

These  larger  nerve-cells  form,  however,  a  part  only,  and  in  most 
regions  of  the  cord  the  smaller  part,  of  the  whole  grey  matter. 
In  a  transverse  section  from  the  thoracic  region  (Fig.  96)  a  few 
only  of  these  larger  nerve-cells  are  seen  in  the  whole  section,  and 
though  they  appear  more  numerous  in  sections  from  the  cervical 
and  especially  from  the  lumbar  regions  (Figs.  98,  99),  yet  in  all 
cases  they  occupy  the  smaller  part  of  the  area  of  the  grey  matter. 
The  larger  part  of  the  grey  matter  consists,  besides  a  neuroglia 

(:  supporting  the  nervous  elements,  of  nerve  filaments  running  in 
various  directions  and  forming,  not  a  plexus  properly  so  called,  but 
an  interlacement  of  extreme  complexity.  These  filaments  are,  on 


II 


862  STRUCTURE   OF   GREY   MATTER.         [BOOK  m. 

the  one  hand,  the  fine  medullated  fibres  spoken  of  above  as  being 
recognized  with  difficulty,  and,  on  the  other  hand,  non-medullated 
filaments  ranging  from  fairly  wide  and  conspicuous  naked  axis- 
cylinders  down  to  fibrils  of  extreme  tenuity,  the  latter  arising 
apparently  either  from  the  division  of  axis-cylinders  of  nerve  fibres 
passing  into  or  out  of  the  grey  matter  or  from  the  continued 
branching  of  processes  of  nerve-cells.  By  the  modes  of  prepara- 
tion now  available  it  has  been  shewn  that  the  fine  medullated  fibres 
so  far  from  being  rare,  are  in  certain  parts  of  the  grey  matter  so 
abundant  as  even  to  preponderate  over  the  non-medullated  fibres 
or  fibrils.  Lastly,  besides  the  conspicuous  nerve-cells  spoken  of 
above,  which,  though  of  various  sizes,  may  all  perhaps  be  spoken 
of  as  large,  a  very  large  number  of  other  cells  of  small  size,  some 
of  which  at  all  events  must  be  regarded  as  true  nerve-cells,  are 
present  in  the  grey  matter. 

The  neuroglia  in  which  all  these  structures,  nerve-cells,  fine 
medullated  nerve  fibres,  naked  axis-cylinders  and  fine  filaments, 
are  imbedded  is  identical  in  its  general  characters  with  that  of 
the  white  matter,  but,  as  naturally  follows  from  the  nature  of  the 
nervous  elements  which  it  supports,  is  differently  arranged.  In- 
stead of  forming  a  system  of  tubular  channels  it  takes  on  the  form 
of  a  sponge- work  with  large  spaces  for  the  larger  nerve-cells  and 
fine  passages  for  the  nervous  filaments.  At  the  junction  of  the 
grey  matter  with  the  white  matter,  the  neuroglia  of  the  one  is 
continuous  with  that  of  the  other,  and  the  connective-tissue  septa 
of  the  latter  run  right  into  the  former ;  the  outline  of  the  grey 
matter  is  not  smooth  and  even,  but  broken  by  tooth-like  processes 
due  to  the  septa.  Since,  as  we  have  just  said,  some  of  the  true 
nerve-cells  are  very  small,  and  since  the  nerve  filaments  like  the 
neuroglia  fibres  are  very  fine  and  take  like  them  an  irregular 
course,  it  often  becomes  very  difficult  in  a  section  to  determine 
exactly  which  is  neuroglia  and  which  are  nervous  elements.  The 
neuroglia  cells  may  however  be  distinguished  perhaps  from  the 
smaller  nerve-cells  by  their  nuclei  not  being  so  conspicuous  or  so 
relatively  large  as  in  a  nerve-cell,  and  by  their  staining  differently. 

The  grey  matter  then  may  be  broadly  described  as  a  bed  of 
neuroglia,  containing  a  certain  number  of  branching  nerve-cells, 
for  the  most  part  though  not  exclusively  large  and  conspicuous, 
but  chiefly  occupied  by  what  is  not  so  much  a  plexus  as  an 
intricate  interweaving  of  nerve  filaments  running  apparently  in 
all  directions.  Some  of  these  filaments  are  fairly  conspicuous 
naked  axis-cylinders,  and  a  few  are  easily  recognized  medullated 
fibres  of  ordinary  size ;  but  by  far  the  greater  number  are  either 
exceedingly  fine  medullated  fibres,  whose  medulla  is  only  made 
evident  by  special  modes  of  preparation  or  delicate  fibrils  devoid 
of  medulla.  With  the  nervous  web  formed  by  these  filaments 
the  branching  processes  of  the  nerve-cells,  on  the  one  hand,  and 
the  divisions  of  nerve  fibres  passing  into  or  out  of  the  grey 


CHAP,  i.]  THE   SPINAL   CORD.  863 

matter,  on  the  other  hand,  appear  to  be  continuous.  It  may  be 
added  that  the  grey  matter  is  well  supplied  with  blood  vessels, 
these  being  in  it,  as  stated  above,  relatively  much  more  numerous 
than  in  the  white  matter. 

§  565.  The  central  canal  is  lined  by  a  single  layer  of  columnar 
epithelial  cells,  which  are  generally  described  as  bearing  cilia; 
but  it  is  not  certain  that  the  processes  which  may  be  seen  project- 
ing from  the  surfaces  of  the  cells  are  really  cilia.  These  epithelial 
cells  rest  not  on  a  distinct  basement  membrane  but  on  a  bed  of 
neuroglia,  free  apparently  or  nearly  so  from  nervous  elements, 
which  surrounds  the  central  canal  and  is  sometimes  spoken  of  as 
the  substantia  gelatinosa  centralis  (Fig.  96,  c.  g.  s.).  The  attached 
bases  of  the  epithelial  cells  are  branched  or  taper  to  a  filament, 
and  become  continuous  with  the  branched  cells  or  fibres  of  the 
neuroglia  below.  As  we  said  above  the  neuroglia  elements  are 
transformed  epithelial  cells ;  and  the  continuity  of  the  cells,  which 
retaining  the  characters  of  epithelial  cells  form  a  lining  to  the 
canal,  with  the  cells  which  have  become  branched  and  lost  their 
epithelial  characters  indicates  the  epithelial  origin  of  the  latter. 

The  central  canal  with  the  surrounding  area  of  neuroglia 
forms  the  central  part  of  the  isthmus  uniting  the  two  lateral 
halves  of  the  cord.  Posterior  (dorsal)  to  this  central  mass  lies  the 
posterior  grey  commissure  (Figs.  96,  98,  99,  p.  g.  c.),  composed 
chiefly  of  fine  filaments  running  transversely,  and  anterior 
(ventral)  to  it  lies  first  the  thinner  anterior  grey  commissure 
(Figs.  96,  98,  99,  a.  g.  c.)  of  a  similar  nature,  and  then  the 
relatively  thick  white  commissure  (Figs.  96,  98,  99,  a.  c.)  which 
is  formed  by  medullated  fibres  crossing  over  from  one  side  of 
the  cord  to  the  other,  and  thus  constitutes  a  decussation  of 
fibres  along  the  whole  length  of  the  cord.  On  each  side,  the 
central  mass  of  neuroglia  of  which  we  are  speaking  gradually 
merges  into  the  central  grey  matter  of  the  corresponding  lateral 
half. 

The  end  or  head  (caput)  as  it  is  frequently  called  of  the 
posterior  horn  is  occupied  not  by  ordinary  grey  matter,  but  by  a 
peculiar  tissue,  the  substantia  gelatinosa  of  Rolando,  which  forms 
a  sort  of  cap  to  the  more  ordinary  grey  matter  but  differs  in 
size  and  shape  in  different  regions  of  the  cord.  Cf.  figs.  96,  98, 
99,  s.g.  In  carmine  and  some  other  modes  of  preparation  it  is 
frequently  stained  more  deeply  than  is  the  ordinary  grey  matter, 
and  in  such  preparations  is  very  conspicuous.  It  may  be  described 
as  consisting  of  a  somewhat  peculiar  neuroglia  traversed  by  fibres 
of  the  posterior  root,  and  containing  a  large  number  of  cells,  which, 
for  the  most  part  small,  the  cell-bodies  being  small  relatively  to 
the  nuclei,  are  not  all  alike,  some  being  probably  nervous  and 
others  not.  It  takes  origin  from  the  cells  forming  the  immediate 
walls  of  the  embryonic  medullary  canal.  In  the  embryo,  this 
canal  is  relatively  wide,  though  compressed  from  side  to  side,  and 
p.  55 


864 


THE   SUBSTANCE   OF   ROLANDO. 


[BOOK 


in  transverse  sections  of  the  medullary  tube  appears  at  a  certain 
stage  as  a  narrow  oval  slit  placed  vertically,  and  reaching  almost 
from  the  dorsal  to  the  ventral  surface.  The  dorsal  part  of  this 
long  slit  is  later  on  closed  up  by  the  coming  together  of  the  walls 
and  the  obliteration  of  the  greater  part  of  the  cavity,  leaving  the 
ventral  part  to  form  a  circular  canal,  which  by  the  development 
of  the  anterior  columns  assumes  the  central  position.  During  this 
closure  of  the  dorsal  part  of  the  canal  a  mass  of  the  cells  lining 
the  canal  is  cut  from  the  rest  on  each  side,  and  during  the  subse- 
quent growth  takes  up  a  position  at  the  end  of  the  posterior  horn. 
Hence,  though  it  never  apparently  contains  any  cavity,  the  sub- 
stance of  Rolando  may  be  regarded  as  an  isolated  portion  of  the 
walls  of  the  medullary  canal,  which  has  undergone  a  development 
somewhat  different  from  that  of  the  portion  which  remains  as 
the  lining  of  the  central  canal.  Traces  of  this  origin  may  be  seen 
even  in  the  adult.  Thus  in  the  lower  end  of  the  cord,  in  what  we 
shall  speak  of  presently  as  the  conus  medullaris,  the  central  canal 
widens  out  dorsally,  and  in  section  (Fig.  97,  A)  presents  on  each 
side  a  bay  x,  stretching  out  towards  the  position  of  the  posterior 
horn.  At  this  region  of  the  cord,  though  both  white  and  grey 
matter  are  developed  on  the  ventral  surface,  the  posterior  columns 
do  not  meet  on  the  dorsal  surface,  but  leave  the  central  canal 
covered  only  by  tissue  which  perhaps  may  be  called  neuroglia,  but 


ABC 

FIG.  97.    DIAGRAM  TO  ILLUSTRATE  THE  NATURE  OF  THE  SUBSTANCE  OF  KOLANDO. 

The  figures  are  purely  diagrammatic  and  are  not  drawn  to  the  same  scale.  In 
all  three  figures  the  grey  matter  is  shaded  with  fine  lines  and  the  white  matter 
with  dots. 

A.  transverse  section  of  the  lower  end  of  the  conus  medullaris  in  man.     e.  epithe- 

lium lining  the  medullary  canal,     x.  lateral  expansion  of  the  canal. 

B.  transverse  section  of  the  spinal  cord  of  the  calf  in  the  lower  thoracic  region. 

r.  substance  of  Eolando.     c.  central  canal. 

C.  transverse  section  through  mid  thoracic  region  of  cord  in  man. 

is  of  peculiar  nature  and  origin.  In  the  calf,  in  a  part  of  the 
dorsal  region  the  substance  of  Rolando  is  not  confined  to  the  tip 
of  the  posterior  horn,  but  is  continued  to  meet  its  fellow  in  the 
middle  line.  Fig.  97,  B.  If  we  imagine  the  dorsal  portion  of  the 
canal  of  A  to  be  cut  off  from  the  ventral  portion,  its  cavity  to  be 
obliterated,  and  the  lining  epithelium  with  some  of  the  sur- 
rounding elements  to  undergo  a  special  development,  the  condition 


CHAP,  i.]  THE   SPINAL   CORD.  865 

in  B  is  reached  by  the  growth  of  the  posterior  columns.  From  B, 
the  transition  to  the  normal  state  of  things  as  in  97,  C,  is  a  very 
slight  one.  The  extreme  dorsal  tip  of  the  horn  being  of  a  more 
open  texture  than  the  substance  of  Rolando,  is  sometimes  called 
the  zona  spongiosa. 

§  566.  The  grouping  of  the  nerve-cells.  The  nerve-cells,  at  all 
events  the  cells  which  are  large  enough  to  be  easily  and  without 
doubt  recognized  to  be  nerve-cells,  form,  as  we  have  seen,  only 
a  part  of  the  grey  matter,  and  in  some  parts  of  the  cord,  in  the 
thoracic  region  for  instance,  are  so  sparse  that  in  a  section  of  the 
spinal  cord  in  this  region  thin  enough  to  shew  its  histological 
features  satisfactorily,  the  bodies  of  a  few  only  of  such  cells  are 
visible  (Fig.  96);  the  greater  part  of  the  grey  matter  consists 
not  of  the  bodies  of  conspicuous  nerve-cells,  but  of  a  mass  of 
fibres  and  fibrils  passing  apparently  in  all  directions.  In  the 
cervical  (Fig.  98)  and  especially  in  the  lumbar  (Fig.  99)  regions 
the  nerve-cells  are  both  absolutely  and  relatively  more  abundant ; 
but  even  in  a  section  taken  from  the  lumbar  region  the  nerve- 
cells,  all  put  together,  torm  the  smaller  part  of  the  whole  area  of 
grey  matter.  Moreover,  in  respect  of  the  number  of  cells  all  the 
sections  of  even  the  same  region  of  the  cord  are  not  alike.  Seeing 
that  the  cord  may  be  considered  as  growing  out  of  the  fusion  of 
a  series  of  paired  ganglia,  each  ganglion  corresponding  to  a  nerve, 
cf.  §  96,  we  may  fairly  expect  to  find  the  fusion  not  complete,  so 
that  the  nerve-cells  would  appear  more  numerous  opposite  a 
nerve  than  in  the  middle  between  two  nerves.  In  some  of  the 
lower  animals  this  arrangement  is  most  obvious,  and  there  are 
some  reasons  for  thinking  that  even  in  man  the  nerve-cells  are 
metamerically  increased  at  the  level  of  each  nerve. 

Even  when  casually  observed  it  is  obvious  that  the  nerve-cells 
are  not  scattered  in  a  wholly  irregular  manner  throughout  the  grey 
matter,  being  for  instance  much  more  conspicuous  in  the  anterior 
horn  than  elsewhere;  and  more  careful  observation  allows  us  to 
arrange  them  to  a  certain  extent  in  groups. 

The  cells  of  the  anterior  horn  are  for  the  most  part  large  and 
conspicuous,  67/z  to  135/4  in  diameter,  branch  out  in  various  direc- 
tions, and  present  an  irregular  outline  in  sections  taken  in  different 
planes.  We  have  reason  to  think  that  every  one  of  them  possesses 
an  axis-cylinder  process  which,  in  the  case  at  all  events  of  most  of 
the  cells,  passing  out  of  the  grey  matter  becomes  a  fibre  of  the 
adjacent  anterior  root.  They  are  obvious  and  conspicuous  in  all 
regions  of  the  cord,  though  much  more  numerous  and  individually 
larger  in  the  cervical  and  lumbar  enlargements  than  in  the  thoracic 
region.  We  may  further,  with  greater  or  less  success,  divide  them 
into  separate  groups. 

In  the  cervical  and  lumbar  regions  a  fairly  distinct  group  of 
cells  is  seen  lying  on  the  median  side  of  the  grey  matter  close  to 
the  anterior  column  (Figs.  98,  99,  1).  This  may  be  called  the 

55—2 


866  THE   NERVE-CELLS   OF   THE   CORD.         [BOOK  m. 


FIG.  98.    TRANSVERSE  DORSOVENTRAL  SECTION  OF  SPINAL  CORD  (HUMAN)  AT 
THE  LEVEL  OF  THE  SIXTH  CERVICAL  NERVE.     (Sherrington.) 

This  is  drawn  on  the  same  scale  as  Fig.  96,  that  is  magnified  15  times. 
r.  /.  1.  lateral  reticular  formation,     r.  f.  p.  posterior  reticular  formation,    p'.  fine 
fibres  of  lateral  bundle  of  the  posterior  root ;  p",  p'"  fibres  of  median  bundle 


CHAP,  i.]  THE   SPINAL   CORD.  867 

of  posterior  root,  entering  grey  matter  from  external  posterior  column,  x.  grey 
matter  of  posterior  horn.  Sp.  a.  bundles  of  fibres  belonging  to  the  spinal 
accessory  nerve;  in  the  lateral  reticular  formation  they  are  seen  cut  trans- 
versely, b.  is  a  natural  septum  of  connective  tissue  marking  out  the  cerebellar 
tract  C.  T.  from  the  crossed  pyramidal  tract  C.P.T.  z.  s.  zona  spongiosa. 
2  a,  j8,  7,  lateral  cells  of  the  anterior  horn.  5.  Cells  in  the  region  of  the  lateral 
reticular  formation.  The  other  letters  of  reference  are  the  same  as  in  Fig.  96. 

median  group.  It  appears  also  in  the  thoracic  region  (Fig.  96,  1) ; 
indeed  the  question  arises  whether  all  the  cells  of  the  anterior 
horn  in  this  region  do  not  belong  to  this  group.  The  other 
cells  so  conspicuous  in  the  lumbar  and  cervical  enlargements, 
and  therefore  probably  in  some  way  associated  with  the  limbs, 
may  be  spoken  of  as  forming  altogether  a  lateral  group ;  but  we 
may,  though  with  some  uncertainty,  subdivide  them  into  two  or 
three  groups.  Thus  in  the  lumbar  region  a  group  of  cells  (Fig. 
99,  27)  lying  near  the  lateral  margin  of  the  more  dorsal  part  or 
base  of  the  horn  may  be  distinguished,  as  a  lateral  subgroup,  from 
the  cells  occupying  the  ventral  lateral  corner  of  the  horn  and 
forming  a  ventral  or  anterior  subgroup  (Fig.  99,  2a);  and  the 
same  distinction,  though  with  less  success,  may  be  made  in  the 
cervical  region  (Fig.  98).  Further,  we  may  perhaps  in  both 
regions  distinguish  a  group  of  cells  placed  more  in  the  very 
middle  of  the  horn  as  a  central  subgroup  (Figs.  98,  99,  2/8).  But, 
in  all  cases,  the  separation  of  these  cells,  which  we  have  spoken 
of  as  a  whole  as  lateral  cells,  into  minor  groups,  is  far  less  distinct 
than  the  separation  of  the  median  group  from  these  lateral  cells, 
especially  if  we  admit  that  in  the  thoracic  region,  the  median 
group  is  alone  clearly  represented. 

In  the  thoracic  region  a  group  of  rather  smaller  cells  is  seen 
at  the  base  of  the  anterior  horn,  near  to  the  junction  with  the 
isthmus  (Fig.  96, 7).  In  the  cervical  and  lumbar  region  these  cells 
are  very  scanty  (Figs.  98,  99,  7). 

The  cells  of  the  posterior  horn  contrast  strongly  with  those  of 
the  anterior  horn  in  being  few,  and  for  the  most  part  small.  They 
are  branched ;  and  though  we  have  reason  to  believe  that,  like  the 
cells  of  the  anterior  horn,  they  possess  each  an  axis-cylinder 
process,  this  is  not  easily  determined  by  actual  observation ;  the 
processes  do  not  run  out  to  join  the  posterior  root  as  do  the  corre- 
sponding processes  in  the  anterior  horn  and  therefore  are  not  so 
readily  seen.  These  cells  occur  in  all  regions  of  the  cord,  and  appear 
to  be  arranged  into  two  more  groups.  The  lateral  margin  of  the 
posterior  horn,  at  about  the  middle  or  neck  of  the  horn,  is  along 
the  whole  length  of  the  cord,  but  especially  in  the  cervical  region, 
much  broken  up  by  bundles  of  fibres  passing  in  various  directions 
and  forming  an  open  network,  called  the  lateral  reticular  formation 
(Figs.  98,  99,  r.  f.  lot.).  In  all  regions  of  the  cord  a  number  of 
cells  are  found  associated  with  this  reticular  formation,  forming 
the  group  of  the  lateral  reticular  formation  (Figs.  98,  99,  5).  In 
all  regions  of  the  cord  also  a  group  of  cells  (Figs.  96,  98,  99,  6) 


868 


THE   NERVE-CELLS   OF   THE   CORD.       [BOOK  in. 


is  found  in  that  part  of  the  horn  where,  a  little  ventral  to  the 
substance  of  Rolando,  the  uniform  field  of  grey  matter  is  broken 
up  into  a  kind  of  network  by  a  number  of  bundles  of  white  fibres 
running  in  various  directions.  This  network  has  also  been  called  a 


- — m.t 


J 


FIG.  99.     TRANSVERSE  DORSOVENTRAL  SECTION  OF  THE  SPINAL  CORD  (HUMAN) 

AT  THE  LEVEL  OF  THE  THIRD  LUMBAR  NERVE.  (Sherrington.) 
This  is  drawn  to  the  same  scale  as  Figs.  96,  97  and  in  the  same  way  except 
that  the  outline  of  the  grey  matter  is  not  exaggerated.  Pr'.  median,  Pr.  inter- 
mediate, Pr".  lateral  bundles  of  posterior  roots.  The  region  comprised  under  m.t. 
is  the  marginal  zone  or  Lissauer's  zone.  The  other  letters  of  reference  are  the 
same  as  in  96  and  98. 

The  three  figures  96,  98,  99  are  intended  to  illustrate  the  main  differential 
features  of  the  thoracic,  cervical,  and  lumbar  cord. 


CHAP,  i.]  THE   SPINAL   CORD.  869 

reticular  formation,  and  has  received  the  name  of  posterior  reticular 
formation  (Figs.  98,  99,  r.  /.  p.)  to  distinguish  it  from  the  lateral 
reticular  formation  just  mentioned ;  the  two  however  in  some 
regions  (see  Fig.  96)  join  each  other,  and  thus  cut  off  a  ventral 
portion  of  the  posterior  horn  containing  nerve-cells  from  a  dorsal 
portion,  x  in  Figs.  98,  99,  in  which  no  obvious  or  conspicuous 
nerve-cells  are  present. 

The  groups  of  cells  just  mentioned  with  the  restrictions  and 
modifications  spoken  of  occur  along  the  whole  length  of  the 
cord ;  but  the  group  of  cells  to  which  we  must  now  call  attention 
is  almost  confined  to  a  special  region  of  the  cord,  or  at  least 
is  but  feebly  represented  elsewhere.  In  the  thoracic  region, 
especially  in  the  lower  thoracic  region  (we  shall  return  to  the 
limits  of  the  group  later  on)  at  the  base  of  the  posterior  horn 
(Fig.  96,  3)  just  ventral  to  the  curve  formed  by  the  posterior  grey 
commissure  as  this  bends  dorsally  to  join  the  posterior  horn,  is 
seen  on  each  side  of  the  cord  a  conspicuous  group  of  cells  known 
as  Clarke's  column  or  the  posterior  vesicular  column  or  vesicular 
cylinder.  The  cells  composing  this  group,  though  varying  in 
size  at  different  levels,  are  rather  large  cells,  and  are  for  the 
most  part  fusiform,  with  their  long  axis  placed  lengthways  along 
the  cord,  so  that  in  transverse  sections  they  often  appear  to  have 
a  rather  small  round  body.  They  are  surrounded  by  and  as  it 
were  imbedded  in  a  mass  of  fine  fibres,  the  area  of  which  is 
indicated  by  a  dotted  line  in  Fig.  96. 

Also  conspicuous  in  the  thoracic  region  is  another  group  of 
cells  lying  on  the  outer  side  of  the  middle  of  the  grey  matter  at 
about  the  junction  of  the  anterior  and  posterior  horns.  This  is 
known  as  the  intermedia-lateral  tract  and  is  sometimes  called 
the  lateral  horn  (Fig.  96,  4).  The  cells  composing  it  are  some- 
what small  spindle-shaped  cells  with  their  long  axis  placed  trans- 
versely. The  group  is  conspicuous  as  we  have  said  in  the  thoracic 
regions ;  it  may  be  recognized  in  the  lumbar  region  (Fig.  99,  4), 
but  in  the  cervical  region  becomes  confused  with  the  most  dorsally 
placed  or  lateral  subgroup  of  the  anterior  horn.  We  shall  however 
have  to  return  to  these  groups  of  cells  when  we  come  to  speak;  of 
the  differences  between  the  several  regions  of  the  cord. 

§  567.  The  tracts  of  white  matter.  At  first  sight  the  white 
matter  of  the  cord  appears  to  be  of  uniform  nature.  We  can  use 
the  nerve  roots  to  delimitate  the  anterior,  posterior  and  lateral 
columns,  but  we  appear  to  have  no  criteria  to  distinguish  parts  in 
each  column.  In  the  cervical  and  upper  thoracic  regions  of  the 
cord,  a  septum  (Fig.  96,  s.)  in  the  posterior  column,  somewhat 
more  conspicuous  than  the  other  septa,  has  enabled  anatomists 
to  distinguish  an  inner  median  portion,  the  median  posterior 
column,  commonly  called  the  poster o-median  column  or  column  of 
Goll  (Fig.  96,  m.  p.),  from  an  outer  lateral  portion,  the  external 
posterior  column,  commonly  called  the  postero-external  column  or 


870  THE   TRACTS   OF   WHITE   MATTER.          [BOOK  in. 

column  of  Burdock  (Fig.  96,  e.  p.\  the  lateral  part  of  which,  nearer 
the  grey  matter,  has,  for  reasons  which  we  shall  see  later  on, 
been  called  the  posterior  root-zone.  But  beyond  this  neither  the 
irregular  septa  nor  other  features  will  enable  us  to  distinguish 
one  part  of  the  white  matter  as  different  in  nature  from  another. 
Nor  have  we  better  success  when  with  the  scalpel  we  attempt  to 
unravel  out  the  white  matter  into  separate  strands.  Nevertheless 
we  have  convincing  evidence  that  the  white  matter  is  arranged  in 
strands,  or  tracts,  or  columns,  which  have  different  connections 
at  their  respective  ends,  which  behave  differently  under  different 
circumstances,  which  we  have  every  reason  to  believe  carry  out 
different  functions,  but  which  cannot  be  separated  by  the  scalpel 
because  each  of  them  is  more  or  less  mixed  with  fibres  of  a 
different  nature  and  origin.  The  evidence  for  the  existence  of 
these  tracts  is  twofold. 

One  kind  of  evidence  is  embryological  in  nature.  When  a 
nerve  fibre  is  being  formed  in  the  embryo,  either  in  the  spinal 
cord  or  elsewhere,  the  essential  axis  cylinder  is  formed  first  and 
the  less  essential  medulla  is  formed  later.  Now  when  the  develop- 
mental history  of  the  spinal  cord  is  studied  it  is  found  that,  in 
the  several  regions  of  the  cord,  all  the  fibres  of  the  white  matter 
do  not  put  on  the  medulla  at  the  same  time.  On  the  contrary, 
in  certain  tracts,  the  medulla  of  the  fibres  makes  its  appearance 
early,  in  others  later.  By  this  method  it  becomes  possible  to 
distinguish  certain  tracts  from  others. 

Another  kind  of  evidence  is  supplied  by  facts  relating  to  the 
degeneration  of  the  fibres  of  the  white  matter.  We  have  seen 
(§  561)  that  the  degeneration  of  a  nerve  fibre  is  the  result  of  the 
separation  of  the  fibre  from  its  trophic  centre,  and  that  while 
the  trophic  centre  of  the  afferent  fibres  is  in  the  ganglion  on  the 
posterior  root,  that  of  the  efferent  fibres  is  in  some  part  of  the 
spinal  cord.  In  the  case  of  the  efferent  fibres  the  degeneration 
might  be  spoken  of  as  descending  from  the  spinal  cord  to  the 
muscles  or  other  peripheral  organs.  In  the  case  of  the  afferent 
fibres  of  the  trunk  of  the  nerve,  the  degeneration  is  also  one 
descending  from  the  ganglion  down  to  the  skin  or  other  peri- 
pheral organ.  When  however  the  section  is  carried  through  the 
posterior  root  of  a  spinal  nerve,  the  degeneration  takes  place  in 
the  part  of  the  nerve  between  the  section  and  the  spinal  cord,  it 
runs  up  from  the  section  to  and  into  the  spinal  cord,  and  may 
therefore  be  called  an  ascending  degeneration.  Thus  we  may  say 
that  when  a  nerve  trunk  or  when  a  nerve  root  is  cut  completely 
across,  all  the  fibres  which  are  thereby  separated  from  their  trophic 
centres,  degenerate.  When  the  nerve  trunk  is  divided  all  the 
fibres  below  the  section  undergo  descending  degeneration.  If 
the  anterior  root  be  cut  across,  all  the  fibres  of  the  root  below  the 
section  undergo  descending  degeneration.  If  the  posterior  root 
be  cut  across,  all  the  fibres  of  the  root  above  the  section  undergo 


CHAP,  i.]  THE   SPINAL   CORD.  871 

ascending  degeneration  with  the  exception  of  certain  fibres  which 
do  not  degenerate  at  all,  and  of  which  we  shall  speak  later  on. 

When  the  spinal  cord  is  cut  across,  for  instance  in  the  dorsal 
region,  all  the  fibres  of  the  white  matter  do  not  degenerate  either 
in  the  part  of  the  cord  above  the  section  or  in  the  part  below. 
Some  fibres,  and  indeed  some  tracts  of  fibres  degenerate,  and  some 
do  not.  Further,  some  tracts  degenerate  in  the  cord  above  the 
section,  and  thus  undergo  what  has  been  called  an  ascending 
degeneration ;  other  tracts  degenerate  in  the  cord  below  the 
section,  and  thus  undergo  what  has  been  called  a  descending 
degeneration.  These  terms  must  however  be  used  with  caution. 
When  a  nerve  trunk  is  cut  across,  the  degeneration  actually 
descends,  in  the  sense  that  the  progress  of  the  degenerative 
changes  may  be  traced  downwards;  they  begin  at  the  section 
and  travel  downwards  at  a  rate  sufficiently  slow  to  permit  a 
difference  being  observed  between  the  progress  of  degeneration  at 
a  spot  near  the  section  and  that  at  one  farther  off.  After  section 
of  or  injury  to  the  spinal  cord,  however,  it  is  not  possible  to 
trace  any  such  progress  either  upwards  or  downwards;  in  the 
tracts  both  above  and  below  the  section  or  injury,  degeneration 
either  begins  simultaneously  along  the  whole  length  of  the 
degenerating  tract,  or  progresses  along  the  tract  so  rapidly 
that  no  differences  can  be  observed  as  far  as  the  stage  of  de- 
generation is  concerned  between  parts  near  to  and  those  far 
from  the  section  or  injury.  When,  for  instance,  the  cord  is 
divided  in  the  cervical  region,  subsequent  examination  of  the 
tracts  of  so-called  descending  degeneration  shews  that  the  de- 
generation is  as  far  advanced  in  the  lumbar  region  far  away 
from  the  section  as  in  the  cervical  region  just  below  the  section. 
Applied  to  the  spinal  cord,  therefore,  the  term  descending  de- 
generation means  simply  degeneration  below  the  seat  of  injury 
or  disease,  ascending  degeneration  means  simply  degeneration 
above  the  seat  of  injury  or  disease.  We  may  add  that  the 
histological  features  of  the  degeneration  of  fibres  in  the  spinal 
cord  are  not  wholly  identical  with  those  of  the  degeneration 
of  fibres  in  a  nerve  trunk.  Thus,  the  neurilemma  with  its  nuclei 
being  absent  from  the  fibres  of  the  cord,  no  proliferation  of  nuclei 
takes  place ;  the  axis-cylinder  and  medulla  simply  break  up,  are 
absorbed  and  disappear. 

Similar  degenerations,  ascending,  or  descending  or  both,  are 
seen  when  the  section  is  not  carried  right  through  the  whole 
cord,  but  particular  parts  of  the  cord  are  cut  through  or  simply 
injured.  And  similar  degenerations  occur  as  the  consequences 
of  disease  set  up  in  parts  of  the  cord. 

In  this  way  the  results  of  sections  of  or  of  other  injuries  to  or 
of  diseases  of  the  spinal  cord  have  enabled  us  to  mark  out  certain 
tracts  of  the  white  matter  as  undergoing  degeneration  and  others 
as  not,  and  moreover  certain  tracts  as  undergoing  descending  and 


872 


THE   TRACTS   OF   WHITE   MATTER.        [BOOK  HI. 


others  as  undergoing  ascending  degeneration.  Further,  the  delimi- 
tation of  tracts  of  white  matter  by  the  process  of  degeneration 
agrees  so  well  with  the  results  of  the  embryological  method  as  to 
leave  no  doubt  that  the  white  matter  does  consist  of  tracts  which 
differ  from  each  other  in  nature  and  in  function. 

The  several  tracts  thus  indicated  vary  in  different  regions 
of  the  cord.  They  may  be  broadly  described  as  follows. 

I.  Descending  tracts,  that  is  to  say,  tracts  which  undergo  a 
descending  degeneration  in  the  sense  noted  above. 

The  most  important  and  conspicuous  is  a  large  tract  (Fig.  100, 
cr.  P.)  occupying  the  posterior  part  of  the  lateral  column,  coming 


sir. 


cr.P 


asc.a.l. 


Cs. 


FIG.  100.     DIAGRAM  TO  ILLUSTRATE  THE  GENERAL  ARRANGEMENT  OF  THE  SEVERAL 
TRACTS  or  WHITE  MATTER  IN  THE  SPINAL  CORD.     (Sherrington.) 

The  section  is  taken  at  the  level  of  the  fifth  cervical  nerve.  The  relations  of 
the  tracts  in  different  regions  of  the  cord  are  shewn  in  Fig.  104. 

The  ascending  tracts,  tracts  of  ascending  degeneration,  are  shaded  with  dots, 
the  descending  tracts,  tracts  of  descending  degeneration,  are  shaded  with  lines; 
the  shading  is  in  each  case  put  on  one  side  of  the  cord  only,  the  reference  letters 
being  placed  on  the  other  side. 

cr.P.  crossed  pyramidal  tract,  or  more  shortly  pyramidal  tract.  d.P.  direct 
pyramidal  tract,  shaded  on  the  side  opposite  to  that  on  which  cr.P.  is  shaded, 
in  order  to  indicate  the  difference  of  the  two  as  to  crossing.  C.b.  cerebellar 
tract,  s.lr.  and  c.r.  together  indicate  the  median  posterior  tract  or  tract  of 
fibres  of  the  posterior  roots,  c.r.  representing,  as  is  explained  more  fully  in  the 
text,  the  cervical  and  s.lr.  the  sacral,  lumbar  and  dorsal  roots,  asc.a.l.  the 
antero-lateral  ascending  tract,  desc.l.  the  antero-lateral  descending  tract. 
The  area,  not  shaded,  marked  x,  is  the  small  descending  tract  or  rather  patch 
mentioned  in  the  text  as  observed,  in  certain  regions  of  the  cord,  in  the 
external  posterior  column  rz.  The  small  area  at  the  tip  of  the  posterior  horn, 
marked  L,  is  the  posterior  marginal  zone  or  Lissauer's  zone. 

close  upon  the  outer  margin  of  the  posterior  horn,  and  for  the 
most  part  not  reaching  the  surface  of  the  cord.  We  shall  have 
to  return  to  this  tract  more  than  once,  and  may  here  simply  say 
that  it  is  most  distinctly  marked  out  by  both  the  embryological 
and  the  degeneration  methods,  that  it  may  be  traced  along  the 
whole  length  of  the  cord  from  the  top  of  the  cervical  region  to 
the  end  of  the  sacral  region,  and  that  it  enters  the  cord  from 


CHAP,  i.]  THE   SPINAL   CORD.  873 

the  brain  through  the  structures  called  the  pyramids  of  the  bulb, 
which  we  shall  study  later  on.  These  pyramids  cross  over  or 
decussate  as  they  are  about  to  pass  into  the  cord,  forming  what  is 
known  as  the  decussation  of  the  pyramids,  and  the  tract  of  fibres 
^  in  question  shares  in  this  decussation.  Hence  this  tract  is  called 
the  crossed  pyramidal  tract  or  more  simply  the  pyramidal  tract. 

A  smaller,  less  conspicuous  descending  tract  occupies  the 
median  portion  of  the  anterior  column  (Fig.  100,  d.  P.).  This 
is  not  only  much  smaller  but  also  much  more  variable  than  the 
crossed  pyramidal  tract,  is  not  present  in  the  lower  animals, 
being  found  in  man  and  the  monkey  only  and  being  better 
developed  in  man  than  in  the  monkey,  and  reaches  a  certain 
way  only  down  the  spinal  cord,  generally  coming  to  an  end  in 
the  thoracic  region.  It  too  comes  down  from  the  pyramid,  and 
is  a  continuation  of  that  part  of  the  pyramid  which  unlike  the 
rest  does  not  decussate  in  the  bulb ;  thus  the  tract  which  coming 
down  from  the  left  side  of  the  brain  runs  in  the  left  pyramid  in 
the  bulb,  passes  down  into  the  left  anterior  column  of  the  cord. 
2-  Hence  this  smaller  tract  is  called  the  direct  pyramidal  tract. 

These  two  are  the  most  conspicuous  and  important  descending 
tracts,  but  names  have  been  given  to  two  other  descending  tracts. 
-%  One,  known  as  the  antero-lateral  descending  tract,  is  a  large 
tract  placed  in  the  antero-lateral  column,  and  seen  in  section 
(Fig.  100,  desc.  I.)  as  an  elongated  area  stretching  from  the  py- 
ramidal tract  towards  the  anterior  column  and  reaching  at  times 
as  far  as  the  anterior  fissure.  The  area  is  large,  however,  because 
the  tract  is  very  diffuse,  that  is  to  say,  the  fibres  with  descending 
degeneration,  or  fibres  which  degenerate  below  the  section  or 
injury,  are  very  largely  mixed  up  with  fibres  which  do  not 
degenerate;  in  this  respect  this  tract  contrasts  with  the  pyra- 
midal tract,  which  is  to  a  much  greater  extent  composed  of 
fibres  with  descending  degeneration,  though  even  in  it  there  are 
a  considerable  number  of  fibres  which  do  not  degenerate.  Indeed 
this  antero-lateral  descending  tract  is  so  diffuse  that  it  hardly 
deserves  to  be  called  a  tract. 

The  other  is  a  small,  narrow,  comma-shaped  tract  (Fig.  100,  x\ 

i      situated  in  the  middle  of  the  external  posterior  column  which  has 

T      been  observed  in  the  cervical  and  upper  thoracic  regions,  and  has 

been  called  the  "  descending  comma  tract."     But  the  degeneration 

reaches  a  short  way  only,  below  the  section  or  injury,  and  the 

group   of  fibres  thus  degenerating  can  hardly  be  considered  as 

forming    a   tract   comparable   to   the    other    tracts.      The    area 

probably  represents   fibres  of  the   posterior  root  which   take   a 

descending  course  soon  after  their  entrance  into  the  cord. 

II.  Ascending  tracts,  that  is  to  say,  tracts  in  which  the 
degeneration  takes  place  above  the  section  or  injury. 

A  conspicuous  ascending  tract  of  a  curved  shape  (Fig.  100, 
G.  b.)  occupies  the  outer  dorsal  part  of  the  lateral  column  lying 


874  THE   TRACTS   OF   WHITE   MATTER.        [BOOK  HI. 

to  the  outside  of  the  crossed  pyramidal  tract,  between  it  and  the 
surface  of  the  cord.  It  appears  to  begin  in  the  upper  lumbar 
region,  being  said  to  be  absent  from  the  lower  lumbar  and  sacral 
cord,  and  may  be  traced  upwards  increasing  in  size  through  the 
thoracic  and  cervical  cord  to  the  bulb.  In  the  bulb  it  may  be 
traced  into  the  restiform  body  or  inferior  peduncle  of  the  cere- 
bellum, and  so  to  the  cerebellum ;  for  the  restiform  body  serves, 
as  we  shall  see,  in  each  lateral  half  of  the  brain,  as  the  main 
connection  of  the  cerebellum  with  the  bulb  and  spinal  cord. 
Hence  this  tract  is  called  the  cerebellar  tract. 

A  second  important  ascending  tract  occupies  the  median 
portion  of  the  posterior  columns  (Fig.  100,  c.r.,  s.lr.),  and  so  far 
coincides  with  what  we  described  above  as  the  median  posterior 
column,  in  the  upper  regions  of  the  cord,  that  it  may  be  called 
the  median  posterior  tract;  it  extends  along  the  whole  length  of 
the  spinal  cord,  varying  at  different  levels  in  a  manner  which  we 
shall  presently  study,  and  ending  above  in  the  bulb. 

A  third  ascending  tract,  called  the  ascending  antero-lateral  tract, 
or  tract  of  Gowers,  occupies  (Fig.  100,  asc.  a.  I.)  the  outer  ventral 
part  of  the  lateral  column.  It  has  somewhat  the  form  of  a 
comma,  with  the  head  filling  up  the  angle  left  between  projecting 
portions  of  the  cerebellar  and  pyramidal  tracts,  and  the  tail 
stretching  away  ventrally  along  the  outer  margin  of  the  lateral 
column  outside  the  antero-lateral  descending  column,  the  end  of 
the  tail  often  reaching  to  the  anterior  roots.  It  may  be  traced 
along  the  whole  length  of  the  cord,  but  is  not  so  distinct  and 
compact  a  tract  as  the  two  ascending  tracts  just  mentioned ;  the 
fibres  with  ascending  degeneration,  that  is  to  say  the  fibres 
degenerating  above  the  section  or  seat  of  injury,  are  very  largely 
mixed  with  fibres  of  a  different  nature  and  origin. 

We  may  further  remark  that  these  several  tracts  differ  from 
each  other,  in  some  cases  markedly,  as  to  the  diameter  of  their 
constituent  fibres.  Thus  the  cerebellar  tract  is  composed  almost 
exclusively  of  remarkably  coarse  fibres.  The  median  posterior 
tract,  on  the  contrary,  is  made  up  of  fine  fibres  of  very  equable  size, 
while  the  fibres  of  the  antero-lateral  ascending  tract  are  of  a  size 
intermediate  between  the  other  two.  The  pyramidal  tract  on  the 
other  hand  is  made  up  of  fibres  of  almost  all  sizes  mixed  together. 

The  tracts  then  which  are  thus  marked  out  are,  as  descending 
tracts,  the  crossed  and  the  direct  pyramidal  tracts,  with  the  less 
distinct  or  important  antero-lateral  descending  tract :  and,  as 
ascending  tracts,  the  cerebellar  tract,  the  median  posterior  tract 
and  the  less  distinct  antero-lateral  ascending  tract.  If  we  suppose 
all  these  tracts  taken  away  there  is  still  left  a  considerable  area  of 
white  matter,  namely,  nearly  the  whole  of  the  external  posterior 
column,  the  external  anterior  column,  including  the  region 
traversed  by  the  bundles  of  the  anterior  roots,  and  that  part  of 
the  lateral  column  which  lies  between  the  antero-lateral  descend- 


CHAP,  i.]  THE   SPINAL   CORD.  875 

ing  tract  and  the  crossed  pyramidal  tract  on  the  outside  and  the 
grey  matter  on  the  inside.  From  this  area  of  white  matter  we 
may  put  on  one  side  at  present  the  external  posterior  column 
because,  as  we  shall  see,  this  column  is  largely  composed  of  the 
fibres  of  the  posterior  root  which  pass  through  this  column, 
especially  through  the  lateral  part  of  it  near  the  grey  matter,  on 
their  way  to  their  ultimate  destination ;  hence  the  alternative 
name  of  posterior  root-zone.  We  may  similarly  leave  for  the 
presQnt  the  small  zone  of  white  matter  composed  of  very  fine 
fibres  known  as  the  posterior  marginal  zone  or  Lissauer's  zone  (Fig. 
100,  L.},  lying  dorsal  to  the  tip  of  the  posterior  horn  and  in  the 
lower  regions  reaching  to  the  outside  of  the  cord ;  for  this  too 
belongs  to  the  fibres  of  the  posterior  root.  Leaving  these  parts 
out  of  consideration  we  may  say  as  regards  the  rest  of  the  white 
matter,  that  the  present  state  of  our  knowledge  will  not  allow  us 
to  divide  it  into  special  tracts.  All  this  area  is  largely  composed 
of  fibres  which  do  not  undergo  either  ascending  or  descending 
degeneration  as  the  result  of  section,  injury  or  disease.  It  has 
been  suggested  that  these  fibres  either  have  no  trophic  centre  at 
all  or  have  double  ones,  one  above  and  one  below,  on  either  of 
which  they  can  in  case  of  need  lean ;  so  that  when  the  fibre  is 
divided  at  any  level,  the  upper  portion  is  still  nourished  from 
some  centre  above,  and  the  lower  from  some  centre  below.  At 
all  events,  whether  this  be  the  true  explanation  or  no,  the  fibres 
in  this  part  of  the  white  matter  cannot  be  differentiated  into 
tracts  by  a  study  of '  their  degeneration.  Fibres  of  this  kind, 
which  we  can  speak  of  neither  as  ascending  nor  as  descending, 
also  occur  in  the  external  posterior  column  mingled  with  the 
fibres  of  the  posterior  root.  And  we  may  repeat  the  caution, 
that  even  in  the  several  ascending  and  descending  tracts  just 
described,  especially  in  those  which  we  spoke  of  as  less  distinct  or 
as  more  diffuse,  many  fibres  are  present  which  undergo  neither 
ascending  nor  descending  degeneration. 

§  568.  It  may  be  as  well  perhaps  to  insist  here  once  more, 
that  when  these  several  tracts  or  the  fibres  running  in  the  tracts 
are  spoken  of  as  ascending  or  descending,  what  is  meant  is  that 
the  degeneration  takes  place  above  the  section  or  seat  of  injury  or 
disease  in  the  one  case,  and  takes  place  below  in  the  other.  It 
has  been  supposed  by  many  that  the  nervous  impulses  which 
these  fibres  severally  carry,  travel  in  the  same  direction  as  that 
taken  by  the  degeneration,  that  the  ascending  tracts  carry  impulses 
from  below  upward,  that  is  to  say,  carry  impulses  which  arising 
from  peripheral  organs  pass  to  various  parts  of  the  spinal  cord  or 
of  the  brain,  that  they  are,  in  other  words,  channels  of  afferent 
impulses,  and  that  conversely  the  descending  tracts  carry  efferent 
impulses.  To  this  view  is  often  added  as  a  corollary,  that  the 
tracts  which  do  not  degenerate  at  all  carry  impulses  both  ways, 
and  hence  cannot  be  considered  as  either  afferent  or  efferent 


876  THE   NERVE   ROOTS.  [BOOK  in. 

channels  but  simply  as  communicating  channels.  Upon  this  it 
may  be  remarked  that  impulses  do  not  necessarily  travel  in  the 
same  direction  as  the  degeneration ;  when  a  spinal  nerve  trunk  is 
divided  the  afferent  fibres  as  well  as  the  efferent  fibres  both 
degenerate  in  a  descending  direction  towards  the  periphery,  though 
the  former  carry  impulses  in  the  other  direction.  Hence  the 
direction  of  degeneration  is  no  proof  of  the  direction  in  which 
impulses  travel ;  moreover,  as  we  have  seen,  degeneration  does  not 
actually  travel  along  the  fibres  of  the  spinal  cord  in  the  same,  way 
that  it  does  along  the  fibres  of  a  nerve  trunk.  It  may  be  that  the 
descending  tracts  do  carry  impulses  in  a  descending  direction,  that 
is,  efferent  impulses,  and  that  the  ascending  tracts  serve  to  carry 
afferent  impulses;  but  the  proof  that  they  do  thus  respectively 
act  must  be  supplied  from  other  facts  than  those  of  degeneration. 
Moreover,  we  shall  have  to  return  to  these  ascending  and  descending 
tracts  and  to  study  their  behaviour  along  the  length  of  the  cord 
before  we  can  use  the  facts  concerning  them  as  a  basis  for  any 
discussion  as  to  their  functions. 

§  569.  The  connections  of  the  nerve  roots.  If  we  regard  the 
spinal  cord,  and  apparently  we  have  right  to  do  so,  as  resulting 
from  the  fusion  of  a  series  of  segments  or  metameres,  each 
segment,  represented  by  a  pair  of  spinal  nerves,  being  a  ganglionic 
mass,  that  is  to  say  a  mass  containing  nerve-cells  with  which  nerve 
fibres  are  connected,  we  should  expect  to  find  that  the  fibres  of  a 
spinal  nerve  soon  after  entering  in,  or  before  issuing  from  the  spinal 
cord  are  connected  with  nerve-cells  lying  in  the  neighbourhood 
of  the  attachment  of  the  nerve  to  the  cord.  We  should,  we  say, 
expect  to  find  this;  but  owing  to  the  difficulty  of  tracing  individual 
nerve  fibres  through  the  tangled  mass  of  the  substance  of  the  cord, 
our  actual  knowledge  of  the  termination  of  the  fibres  of  the 
posterior  root,  and  origin  of  the  fibres  of  the  anterior  root  is  at 
present  far  from  complete. 

With  regard  to  the  anterior  root,  there  can  be  no  doubt  that 
a  very  large  proportion  of  the  fibres  in  the  root  are  continuations 
of  the  axis-cylinders  of  cells  in  the  anterior  horn.  The  fibres 
which  can  thus  be  traced  are  of  large  diameter  and  appear  to  be 
chiefly  if  not  exclusively  motor  fibres  for  the  skeletal  muscles.  In 
the  frog  a  laborious  enumeration  on  the  one  hand  of  the  number 
of  fibres  in  the  anterior  roots,  arid  on  the  other  hand  of  the 
number  of  cells  of  the  anterior  horn  in  the  areas  corresponding 
to  the  nerve  roots  has,  it  is  true,  shewn  a  very  remarkable 
agreement  in  number  between  the  two.  We  might  be  inclined 
from  this  to  conclude  that  all  the  fibres  of  an  anterior  root  start 
directly  from  cells  in  the  anterior  horn,  and  that  all  the  cells  in 
the  anterior  horn  end  in  fibres  of  the  nearest  anterior  root. 
But  several  considerations  prevent  us  from  trusting  too  much  to 
this  observation,  especially  in  the  case  of  the  higher  animals. 
The  anterior  root  contains  other  fibres  than  motor  fibres  for  the 


CHAP,  i.]  THE   SPINAL   CORD.  877 

skeletal  muscles,  vaso-motor  fibres  for  instance,  secretory  fibres  and 
others ;  and  it  is  a  priori  unlikely  that  these  should  have  origin 
from  the  same  cells  as  the  motor  fibres  of  the  skeletal  muscles. 
Moreover,  as  a  matter  of  fact  some  of  the  fibres  have  been  traced 
through  the  anterior  horn,  on  the  one  hand  towards  the  posterior 
horn  and  on  the  other  hand  towards  the  lateral  column;  others 
again  are  found  to  pass  through  the  anterior  horn  of  their  own 
side  to  the  bottom  of  the  anterior  fissure  where,  crossing  over  to 
the  other  side  and  thus  forming  part  of  the  anterior  white  com- 
missure, they  appear  to  ascend  to  the  anterior  horn  of  the  other 
side.  We  cannot  at  present  make  any  positive  statement  as  to  the 
real  origin  and  exact  nature  of  these  fibres  which  thus  upon 
entering  the  cord  pass  by  the  cells  in  the  anterior  horn  without 
joining  them,  though  those  which  cross  by  the  anterior  white 
commissure  are  supposed  to  take  origin  in  the  cells  of  the  anterior 
horn  of  the  other  side ;  it  is  sufficient  for  our  present  purposes  to 
remember  that  while  a  large  number  of  the  fibres  of  the  anterior' 
root,  presumably  those  supplying  the  skeletal  muscles,  take  origin 
in  the  cells  of  the  anterior  horn,  shortly  before  they  issue  from  the 
cord,  others  have  some  other  origin.  And  similarly  we  have  reason 
to  think  that  all  the  cells  in  the  anterior  horn  do  not  send  out 
axis-cylinder  processes  to  join  the  anterior  roots  of  the  same  side. 
We  may  however  regard  a  large  number  at  all  events  of  the  cells 
of  the  anterior  horn,  at  the  level  of  as  well  as  a  little  below  and  a 
little  above  the  level  of  the  exit  of  any  particular  anterior  root,  as  , 
constituting  a  sort  of  nucleus  of  origin  for  the  larger  number  of 
the  fibres,  and  those  most  probably  the  skeletal  motor  fibres,  of 
that  anterior  root. 

The  posterior  root  enters  the  cord  not  in  several  bundles  D 
laterally  scattered  as  does  the  anterior  root,  but  in  a  more 
compact  mass.  This  mass  however  consists  of  at  least  two 
distinct  bundles,  which  upon  their  entrance  into  the  cord,  take 
different  courses.  One  bundle,  the  larger  one,  lying  to  the  inner 
or  median  side  of  the  other,  consisting  of  relatively  coarse  fibres, 
and  called  the  median  bundle  (Fig.  98,  P.r'),  passes  obliquely  into 
the  lateral  part  of  the  external  posterior  column,  which,  as  we 
have  said,  is  in  consequence  often  spoken  of  as  the  posterior 
root-zone.  Here  the  fibres  changing  their  direction  run  longi- 
tudinally for  some  distance  upwards  (some  however,  certainly  in 
the  upper  cervical  region,  and  probably  in  other  regions,  run  a 
short  distance  downwards)  but  eventually  either  go,  as  we  shall 
see,  to  form  the  median  posterior  tract  or  make  their  way  back 
into  the  grey  matter  at  the  base  of  the  posterior  horn  and  thus 
join  the  vesicular  cylinder,  though  some  are  said  to  be  continued 
on  through  the  grey  matter  into  the  anterior  horn.  The  other 
smaller  bundle  placed  to  the  outside  of  the  former,  and  called  the 
lateral  bundle  (Fig.  98,  P.r),  may  be  again  divided  into  an  inter- 
mediate bundle  (Fig.  99,  Pr)  lying  next  to  the  median  bundle, 


878  THE   NERVE   ROOTS.  [BOOK  m. 

and  into  a  still  more  lateral  bundle  (Fig.  99,  Pr").  The  former, 
consisting  also  of  coarse  fibres,  plunges  directly  through  the  sub- 
stance of  Rolando  at  the  extremity  of,  and  so  into  the  grey  matter 
of  the  horn,  where  the  fibres  changing  their  direction  run  in  part 
at  least  longitudinally  in  the  grey  matter  in  bundles  known  as 
"the  longitudinal  bundles  of  the  posterior  horn"  Figs.  98,  99 
r.  f  p.  some  of  which  appear  to  pass  on  to  the  anterior  horn. 
The  small  most  external  or  lateral  portion  of  the  lateral  bundle, 
consisting  of  fine  fibres  and  sometimes  spoken  of  as  the  lateral 
bundle,  on  entering  the  cord  at  once  ascends  for  some  distance, 
and  thus  forms  the  thin  layer  of  fine  fibres,  the  posterior  marginal 
zone  or  Lissauer's  zone,  indicated  in  Fig.  99  by  m.  t,  which  lies 
between  the  actual  extremity  of  the  horn  and  the  surface  of  the 
cord,  and  in  the  upper  regions  of  the  cord  (cf.  Fig.  98,  p)  runs 
some  way  upward  on  the  lateral  margin  of  the  horn  between  the 
grey  matter  and  the  crossed  pyramidal  tract.  As  it  ascends  this 
layer  continually  gives  off  fibres  to  the  grey  matter  of  the 
posterior  horn  in  the  cells  of  which  they  appear  to  end. 

Thus,  while  part  of  the  median  bundle  does  not  join  the  grey 
matter  at  all  but  goes  to  form  the  median  posterior  tract,  the  rest 
of  that  bundle  and  all  the  other  fibres  of  the  root,  sooner  or  later, 
join  the  grey  matter  either  of  the  posterior  horn  or  of  some  other 
part. 

§  570.  The  Special  Features  of  the  several  regions  of  the  Spinal 
Cord.  The  cord  begins  below  in  the  slender  filament  called  the 
filum  terminate,  which  lying  in  the  vertebral  canal,  in  the  midst  of 
the  mass  of  nerve  roots  called  the  cauda  equina,  rapidly  enlarges 
at  about  the  level  of  the  first  lumbar  vertebra  into  the  conus 
medullaris.  This  may  be  regarded  as  the  beginning  of  the  lower 
portion  of  a  fusiform  enlargement  of  the  cord  known  as  the  lumbar 
swelling,  which  reaches  as  high  as  about  the  attachment  of  the 
roots  of  the  twelfth  or  eleventh  thoracic  nerve  at  the  level  of  the 
eighth  thoracic  vertebra,  the  broadest  part  of  the  swelling  being 
about  opposite  the  third  lumbar  nerve.  Above  the  lumbar 
swelling,  through  the  thoracic  region  the  somewhat  narrowed  cord 
retains  about  the  same  diameter  until  it  reaches  the  level  of 
the  first  or  second  thoracic  nerve  opposite  the  seventh  cervical 
vertebra  where  a  second  fusiform  enlargement,  the  cervical  swelling, 
broader  and  longer  than  the  lumbar  swelling,  begins.  The  broadest 
part  of  the  cervical  swelling  is  about,  opposite  to  the  fifth  or  sixth 
cervical  nerve ;  from  thence  the  diameter  of  the  cord  becomes 
gradually  somewhat  less  until  it  begins  to  expand  into  the  bulb, 
but  even  in  the  highest  part  is  greater  than  in  the  thoracic  region. 
The  sectional  area  of  the  cord  increases  therefore  from  below 
upwards,  but  not  regularly,  the  irregularity  being  due  to  the 
lumbar  and  cervical  swellings. 

The  extremity  of  the  filum  terminale  is  said  to  consist  entirely 
of  neuroglia  closely  invested  by  the  membranes,  even  the  central 


CHAP,  i.]  THE   SPINAL   CORD.  879 

canal  being  absent.  A  little  higher  up  the  central  canal  begins, 
and  nerve-cells  with  nerve-fibres  make  their  appearance  in  the 
neuroglia;  thus  a  kind  of  grey  matter  covered  by  a  thin  super- 
ficial layer  of  white  matter  is  established.  We  have  already 
referred  to  the  peculiar  features  of  the  lower  end  of  the  conus 
§  565 ;  but  higher  up  the  canal  becomes  central  and  small,  the 
posterior  columns  are  developed,  and  the  grey  matter  contains 
more  nervous  elements  and  relatively  less  neuroglia,  becomes  in 
fact  ordinary  grey  matter.  From  thence  onward  to  very  near  the 
junction  with  the  bulb,  where  transitional  features  begin  to  come 
in,  the  spinal  cord  may  be  said  to  have  the  general  structure 
previously  described. 

The  sectional  area  of  the  white  matter  increases  in  absolute 
size  and  on  the  whole  in  a  steady  manner  from  below  upwards. 
In  other  words,  in  a  section  at  any  level,  the  number  of  longi- 
tudinal fibres  forming  the  white  matter  is  greater  than  the 
number  at  a  lower  level,  and  less  than  the  number  at  a  higher 
level ;  for  any  difference  which  may  exist  in  the  diameter  of  the 
individual  fibres  is  insufficient  to  explain  the  differences  in  the 
total  sectional  area  of  the  white  matter.  If  we  were  to  measure  in 
man  the  sectional  area  of  each  of  the  spinal  nerves  as  it  joins  the 
cord,  and  to  add  them  together,  passing  along  the  cord  from  below 


V    IV  III    II    I    V    IV  III   II     I    X.I  XI    X   IX  VIII VII  VI    V    IV   III    II    I   VIII VII  VI  V   IV  III    II     I 

FlG.    101.     DIAGRAM  SHEWING  THE    UNITED    SECTIONAL   AREAS    OF   THE    SPINAL  NERVES, 
PROCEEDING   FROM    BELOW    UPWARDS. 

In  this  as  in  the  succeeding  figures  102 — 3,  — 5,  — 6,  — 7,  all  of  which  refer  to 
man,  the  left-hand  side  represents  the  bottom  of  the  cord  and  the  right-hand  the 
top  of  the  cord,  the  numerals  indicating  successively  the  sacral,  lumbar,  thoracic 
and  cervical  nerves.  The  several  figures  are  not  drawn  to  the  same  scale. 

upwards  the  results  put  in  the  form  of  a  curve  would  give  us 
some  such  figure  as  that  shewn  in  Fig.  101 ;  the  area  gained 
by  adding  together  the  sectional  areas  of  the  nerves  increases 
in  a  fairly  steady  manner  from  below  upwards.  The  curve  of 
the  sectional  area  of  the  white  matter  of  the  cord  taken  from 
below  upwards  would  be  very  similar,  but  if  anything  more 
regular.  It  must  be  understood  however  that  the  dimensions  of 
the  areas  would  not  be  the  same  in  the  two  cases.  The  sectional 
area  of  the  white  matter  at  the  top  of  the  cervical  region,  though 
greater  than  anywhere  lower  down,  is  far  less  than  the  united 
sectional  area  of  all  the  nerves  below  that  level.  The  white 
F.  56 


880    THE  FEATURES  OF  DIFFERENT  REGIONS.     [BOOK  in. 


matter  is  not  formed  by  all  the  fibres  from  the  nerves  which  join 
the  spinal  cord  continuing  to  run  along  the  cord  up  to  the  brain ; 
as  we  have  seen,  some  at  least  of  the  fibres  end  in  the  grey 
matter.  Nevertheless  the  white  matter  in  passing  up  the  cord 
appears  to  receive  a  permanent  addition  at  the  entrance  of  each 
nerve.  We  may  infer  that  each  nerve  has  a  representative  of 
itself  starting  from  the  level  of  its  entrance  and  running  up  to 
some  part  of  the  brain.  Whether  the  fibres  thus  representative 
of  the  nerve  are  continuations  of  the  very  fibres  of  the  nerve 
itself,  or  are  new  fibres  starting  from  some  relay  of  grey  matter, 
with  which  the  fibres  of  the  nerve  are  also  connected,  is  another 
question. 

§  571.  The  grey  matter  in  contrast  to  the  white  matter 
shews  great  variations  in  area  along  the  length  of  the  cord  (Fig. 
102).  From  the  entrance  of  the  coccygeal  nerve  upwards  the  area 


V  IV  III   II    I    V   IV  III  II    I  XII  XI  X  IX  VIII  VII  VI  V  IV  III   II    I  VIII  VII  VI  V  IV  III   II    I 

FIG.  102.    DIAGRAM  SHEWING  THE  VARIATIONS  IN  THE  SECTIONAL  AREA  OF  THE  GREY 
MATTER  OF  THE  SPINAL  CORD,  ALONG  ITS  LENGTH. 

increases  very  rapidly,  reaching  a  maximum  at  about  the  level  of 
the  5th  lumbar  nerve.  It  then  rapidly  decreases  to  about  the  level 
of  the  llth  thoracic  nerve,  maintains  about  the  same  dimensions  all 
through  the  thoracic  region,  and  begins  to  increase  again  at  about 
the  level  of  the  2nd  thoracic  nerve.  Its  second  maximum  is 
reached  at  about  the  level  of  the  5th  or  6th  cervical  nerve,  after 
which  the  area  again  becomes  smaller,  remaining  however  at  the 
upper  cervical  region  much  larger  than  in  the  thoracic  region. 
The  meaning  of  these  variations  becomes  clear  when  we  turn 


V  IV  III   II    I   V  IV  III    II    I  XII  XI  X  IX  VIII  VII  VI  V  IV  III  II    I  VIII  VII  VI  V  IV  III   >l    I 

FIG.  103.    DIAGRAM  SHEWING  THE  RELATIVE  SECTIONAL  AREAS  OF  THE  SPINAL  NERVES, 
AS  THEY  JOIN  THE  SPINAL  CORD. 

to  Fig.  103,  which  shews  in  a  similar  diagrammatic  manner  the 
sectional  areas  of  the  several  spinal  nerves.  It  will  be  observed 
that  the  increase  and  decrease  of  the  sectional  area  of  the  grey 
matter  follow  very  closely  the  increase  and  decrease  of  the  quantity 


CHAP,  i.]  THE   SPINAL   CORD.  881 

of  nerve,  that  is  to  say,  neglecting  differences  in  the  diameter  of 
the  fibres,  in  the  number  of  nerve-fibres  passing  into  the  cord. 
The  sectional  areas  of  the  1st  and  2nd  sacral,  4th  and  5th  lumbar 
nerves  are  very  large,  and  opposite  to  these  the  sectional  area  of 
the  grey  matter  of  the  cord  is  very  large  also  ;  the  enlargement  of 
grey  matter  which  is  the  essential  cause  of  the  lumbar  swelling  is 
correlated  to  the  large  number  of  fibres  which  enter  and  leave  the 
cord  at  this  region  to  supply  chiefly  the  lower  limbs.  Similarly 
the  enlargement  of  grey  matter  which  is  the  essential  cause  of  the 
cervical  swelling  is  correlated  to  the  large  number  of  fibres  which 
enter  and  leave  this  region  of  the  cord  to  supply  chiefly  the  upper 
limbs.  In  the  thoracic  region,  where  the  number  of  fibres  entering 
and  leaving  the  cord  is  relatively  less,  the  sectional  area  of 
the  grey  matter  is  also  less.  Since  the  attachments  of  the 
several  spinal  nerves  are  not  exactly  equidistant  from  each  other 
along  the  length  of  the  cord,  the  sectional  area  is  not  an  exact 
measure  of  bulk;  the  total  bulk  of  grey  matter  for  instance 
belonging  to  two  nerves  which  enter  the  cord  close  together  is  less 
than  that  of  two  nerves  giving  rise  to  the  same  sectional  area  of 
grey  matter  as  the  former  two  but  entering  the  cord  far  apart 
from  each  other.  Still  the  error  which  may  be  introduced  by 
taking  sectional  area  to  mean  bulk  is,  for  present  purposes  at  all 
events,  so  small  that  we  may  permit  ourselves  to  say  that  in  the 
successive  regions  of  the  spinal  cord  the  bulk  of  grey  matter  in 
any  segment  is  greater  or  less  according  to  the  size  of  the  nerve  (or 
pair  of  nerves,  right  and  left)  belonging  to  that  segment. 

From  this  anatomical  fact  we  appear  justified  in  drawing  the 
conclusion  that  at  all  events  a  great  deal  of  the  grey  matter  of  the 
spinal  cord  may  be  considered  as  furnishing  a  nervous  mechanism, 
with  which  the  efferent  fibres  of  each  spinal  nerve  just  before 
they  leave  the  cord,  and  the  afferent  fibres  soon  after  they  join 
the  cord  are  more  immediately  connected.  It  may  be  that  the 
whole  of  the  grey  matter  is  thus  directly  connected  with  and  thus 
rises  and  falls  with  the  fibres  of  the  nerves;  or  it  may  be  that  there 
is  a  sort  of  core  of  grey  matter,  which  maintains  a  uniform  bulk 
along  the  whole  length  of  the  cord  and  serves  as  a  basis  which 
is  here  more  and  there  less  swollen  by  the  addition  of  the  grey 
matter  more  immediately  connected  with  the  fibres  of  the  nerves. 
This  question  the  method  which  we  are  now  using  cannot  settle. 

§  572.  Owing  to  these  different  rates  of  increase  of  the  grey 
and  white  matter  respectively  along  the  length  of  the  cord,  we 
find  that  in  sections  of  the  cord  taken  at  different  levels  the 
appearances  presented  vary  in  a  very  distinct  manner.  This  is 
strikingly  shewn  by  comparing  Figs.  96,  98  and  99.  At  the  level 
of  the  third  lumbar  nerve  (Fig.  99)  the  grey  matter  is  very  large, 
reaching,  as  we  have  seen,  its  maximal  sectional  area  at  about  this 
point,  so  that  although  the  area  of  white  matter  is  not  very  great 
the  whole  area  of  the  cord  is  considerable. 

56—2 


882     THE  FEATURES  OF  DIFFERENT  REGIONS.     [BOOK  in. 


At  the  level  of  the  sixth  thoracic  nerve  (Fig.  96),  in  spite  of 
the  white  matter  having  very  decidedly  increased,  the  grey  matter 
has  shrunk  to  such  very  small  dimensions,  that  the  total  sectional 
area  of  the  cord  has  markedly  diminished. 

At  the  level  of  the  sixth  cervical  (Fig.  98)  the  grey  matter 
has  again  increased,  reaching  here  as  we  have  seen  its  second 
maximum ;  the  white  matter  has  also  further  increased,  and  that 
indeed  very  considerably,  so  that  the  total  area  of  the  cord  is 
much  greater  than  in  any  of  the  lower  regions. 

Further  details  of  the  varying  size  of  the  white  matter  and  of 
the  grey  matter  at  different  levels  are  also  shewn  in  the  series  given 


c.a.l. 


C8. 


CHAP,  i.] 


THE   SPINAL   CORD. 


883 


Da 


Sac. 


FIG.  104.     DIAGRAM  ILLUSTRATING   SOME   OF  THE  FEATURES  OF  THE   SPINAL   COBD 
AT  DIFFERENT  LEVELS.     (Sherrington.) 

All  the  figures  are  drawn  to  scale,  and  represent  the  cord  magnified  four 
times.  They  shew  the  differences  at  different  levels  in  the  shape  and  size  of  the 
cord,  in  the  outline  of  the  grey  matter,  and  in  the  relative  position  of  the  anterior 
and  posterior  fissures,  and  also  shew  the  variations  at  different  levels  of  the  several 
*  tracts  '  of  the  white  matter. 

C2  at  the  level  of  the  second  cervical  nerve,  C5  of  the  fifth  cervical,  Cs  of  the  eighth 
cervical.  D2  of  the  second  thoracic,  D5  of  the  fifth  thoracic,  Lx  of  the  first 
lumbar,  LB  of  the  fifth  lumbar,  and  Sac.  of  the  second  sacral  nerve. 

The  shading  of  the  tracts  is  the  same  as  in  Fig.  100;  but  in  the  median  posterior 
column  of  D2  the  areas  of  fibres  coming  from  the  sacral  nerves  s.r.,  and  lumbar 
nerves  I.  r.  are  distinguished  from  the  area,  d.  r.  of  fibres  belonging  to  the 
thoracic  nerves.  In  C  8,  no  distinction  is  made  between  any  of  these  sets 
of  fibres;  in  L5  only  fibres  of  sacral  nerves  are  represented;  in  Lj  D8  D5,  the 
more  dorsal  small  portion  corresponds  to  sacral  fibres  and  the  next  to  lumbar, 
or  lumbar  thoracic  nerves. 

in  Fig.  104.    In  these,  combined  with  the  three  figures  just  referred 
to,  it  will  be  observed  that  the  serial  increase  and  decrease  of  the 


884     THE  FEATURES  OF  DIFFERENT  REGIONS.     [BOOK  m. 

grey  matter  does  not  affect  all  parts  of  the  grey  matter  alike,  so  that 
the  outline  of  the  grey  matter  changes  very  markedly  in  passing 
from  below  upwards.  In  the  coccygeal  region  each  lateral  half  is 
a  somewhat  irregular  oval,  and  in  the  sacral  region,  Fig.  104,  Sac, 
the  differentiation  into  anterior  and  posterior  horns  is  still  very 
indistinct.  In  the  lumbar  region  the  two  horns  are  sharply  marked 
out,  though  both  the  posterior  and  anterior  horns  are  broad  and 
more  or  less  quadrate.  In  the  thoracic  region  the  decrease  of 
grey  matter  has  affected  both  horns,  so  that  both  are  pointed  and 
slender,  while  the  junction  between  them  has  not  undergone  so 
much  diminution,  so  that  what  has  been  called  the  lateral  horn 
is  relatively  conspicuous.  In  the  cervical  region  the  returning 
increase  bears  much  more  on  the  anterior  horn  which  again  becomes 
large  and  broad,  than  on  the  posterior  horn  which  still  remains 
slender  and  pointed.  Taking  the  form  of  the  grey  matter  in  the 
thoracic  region  as  the  more  typical  form  of  the  grey  matter  we 
may  say  that  while  the  increase  on  the  lumbar  swelling  bears 
equally  on  the  anterior  and  posterior  horns,  that  in  the  cervical 
region  bears  chiefly  on  the  anterior  horns. 

Now  we  have  no  reason  to  suppose  that  either  afferent 
impulses  reach  the  lumbar  spinal  cord  in  greater  numbers  from 
the  lower  limbs,  or  along  any  of  the  nerves  joining  this  part  of 
the  cord,  or  that  those  which  do  reach  it  are  of  a  more  complex 
nature  than  is  the  case  with  the  afferent  impulses  reaching  the 
cervical  cord  along  the  nerves  of  the  upper  limbs.  The  increase 
of  grey  matter  in  the  posterior  horns  is  therefore  not  correlated 
to  any  increase  in  the  number  or  complexity  of  the  afferent 
impulses  reaching  the  cord;  and  we  may,  provisionally,  conclude 
that  at  least  a  large  part  of  the  grey  matter  in  the  posterior 
horn  is  not  specially  concerned  in  any  elaboration  or  transformation 
of  afferent  impulses  immediately  upon  their  arrival  at  the  cord. 
Indeed  we  have  seen  that  while  there  is  ample  evidence  to  connect 
the  nerve  cells,  and  therefore  presumably  the  grey  matter  in 
general  of  the  anterior  horn  with  the  efferent  motor  fibres  of  the 
anterior  root,  there  is  no  corresponding  evidence  as  to  any  large 
immediate  connection  of  the  afferent  fibres  of  the  posterior  root 
with  the  nerve  cells  or  indeed  any  other  part  of  the  grey  matter  of 
the  posterior  horn.  We  may  add  that,  as  we  shall  point  out  later 
on,  so  essential  is  the  concurrence  of  appropriate  afferent  impulses 
to  the  due  carrying-out  of  complex  coordinate  motor  or  efferent 
impulses,  that  we  can  scarcely  expect  to  find  any  increase  in  the 
nervous  mechanisms  devoted  to  the  purely  motor  function  of 
carrying  out  motor  impulses  without  a  corresponding  increase  in 
the  nervous  mechanisms  belonging  to  the  afferent  impulses,  by 
means  of  which  those  motor  impulses  are  guided  and  coordinated. 
Hence,  were  the  latter  nervous  mechanisms  restricted  to  the 
posterior  horns  we  should  expect  to  find  a  greater  parallelism  than 
does  actually  exist  between  them  and  the  anterior  horns. 


CHAP,  i.]  THE   SPINAL   CORD.  885 

§  573.  The  changes  in  the  area  of  grey  matter  illustrated  by 
the  statements  and  diagrams  given  above  refer  to  the  grey  matter 
as  a  whole,  that  is,  not  only  to  nerve  cells,  but  also  to  strands 
and  networks  of  nerve  fibres  and  nerve  fibrils,  and  indeed  include 
to  a  certain  extent  neuroglia.  We  have  seen  §  566  that  we  are 
able  to  distinguish  certain  large  and  conspicuous  nerve  cells  in 
the  grey  matter  and  to  arrange  these  into  groups.  The  grey 
matter  contains  many  other  small  nerve  cells,  which  we  are  not 
able  at  present  to  name  or  arrange,  but  whose  existence  must 
always  be  borne  in  mind.  Confining  ourselves  now  however  to 
the  groups  of  larger,  more  conspicuous  nerve  cells,  we  find  that, 
broadly  speaking,  the  chief  differences  which  can  be  observed  in 
the  cells  of  the  anterior  horn  along  the  length  of  the  cord  are 
that  in  the  thoracic  region  the  nerve  cells  of  the  anterior  horn 
are  few,  and  relatively  small,  while  in  the  cervical  and  lumbar 
region,  especially  in  the  latter,  they  are  numerous  and  large.  It 
is  not  easy,  even  if  possible,  to  distinguish  in  the  thoracic  negion 
the  several  groups  of  cells  marked  in  Figs.  98,  99  as  2a,  yS,  7 ;  the 
median  group  (Figs.  98,  99,  1),  indeed  seems  to  be  the  only  group 
present  in  the  mid  thoracic  region  (Fig.  96,  1).  The  group  of  the 
posterior  horn  (Figs.  96,  98,  99,  6)  appears  to  be  about  the  same 
in  all  regions. 

With  two  other  groups  of  nerve  cells  striking  differences  are 
seen  in  different  regions.  The  vesicular  cylinder,  for  instance 
(Fig.  96,  3),  is  most  conspicuous  in  the  thoracic  region.  It  may 
be  said  to  reach  from  the  7th  or  8th  cervical  nerve  to  the  3rd 
lumbar  nerve,  being  perhaps  most  developed  in  the  lower  thoracic 
and  upper  lumbar  region.  It  is  absent  in  the  cervical  region 
above  the  7th  or  8th  cervical  nerve,  and  in  the  lumbar  region 
below  the  3rd  lumbar  nerve ;  but  a  similar  group  of  cells  is 
present  opposite  the  2nd  and  3rd  cervical  nerves ;  a  group  of 
more  doubtful  likeness  is  seen  in  the  sacral  region  below;  and 
the  column  is  said  to  have  a  representative  in  the  bulb  above 
the  spinal  cord  proper.  It  seems  natural  to  infer  that  the  cells 
forming  this  vesicular  cylinder  are  connected  neither  with  the 
ordinary  somatic  motor  fibres  governing  the  skeletal  muscles,  nor 
with  the  ordinary  afferent  sensory,  somatic  fibres  coming  from  the 
skin  and  elsewhere,  but  in  some  way  with  some  special  sets  of 
fibres;  on  this  point  however  no  authoritative  statement  can  as 
yet  be  made. 

The  lateral  horn  or  intermedio-lateral  tract  Fig.  96,  4  is 
also  most  conspicuous  in  the  thoracic  region.  In  the  lumbar 
region,  it  is  lost  or  traced  with  great  difficulty,  and  in  the  cervical 
region  seems  to  be  merged  into  the  most  dorsally  placed  division 
of  the  lateral  group  of  the  cells  of  the  anterior  horn.  It  is  possible 
that  this  group  represents  in  the  limbless  thoracic  region  the  cells 
which  are  developed  into  the  great  lateral  group  of  the  anterior 
horn  in  the  regions  of  the  limbs. 


886     THE  FEATURES  OF  DIFFERENT  REGIONS.     [BOOK  in. 

§  574.  The  white  matter  as  we  have  seen  increases  in  sectional 
area  with  considerable  regularity  from  below  upwards.  If  instead 
of  a  diagram  of  the  increase  of  the  whole  white  matter,  we 
construct  in  a  similar  way  diagrams  of  the  anterior,  posterior  and 


V    IV    III     II     I      V    IV    III     II      I    XII  XI    X    IX  VIII  VII  VI     V    IV    III    II      I    VIII   VII  VI    V 


FIG.    105.    DIAGRAM    SHEWING    THE   VARIATIONS    IN   THE   SECTIONAL  AREA   OF  THK 

LATERAL   COLUMNS   OF   THE    SPINAL    CORD,    ALONG   ITS   LENGTH. 


V    IV  111    II      I     V    IV   III     II      I    XII  XI   X    IX  VIII  VII  VI    V    IV    111     II     I    VIII  VII  VI    V    IV    III    l|     | 


FIG.   106.    DIAGRAM   SHEWING    THE    VARIATIONS    IN  THE   SECTIONAL  AREA    OF  THE 
ANTERIOR  COLUMNS  OF  THE  SPINAL  CORD,  ALONG  ITS  LENGTH. 


V    IV    III 


I     V    IV 


XII  XI     X    IX  VIII  VII   VI    V     IV   III     II     I   VIII   VII  VI    V    IV    III    II     I 


FIG.    107.    DIAGRAM    SHEWING   THE    VARIATIONS  IN  THE   SECTIONAL    AREA  OF  THE 

POSTERIOR   COLUMNS   OF   THE    SPINAL    CORD,    ALONG   ITS   LENGTH. 


lateral  columns  respectively  we  find  that  while  the  sectional  area 
of  the  lateral  column  (Fig.  105)  increases  with  some  considerable 
regularity  from  below  upwards,  though  not  so  regularly  as  does 
the  whole  area  of  white  matter,  both  the  anterior  (Fig.  106)  and 
the  posterior  (Fig.  107)  columns  agree  to  a  certain  extent  with 
the  grey  matter  in  shewing  a  decided  increase  in  both  the  lumbar 
and  the  cervical  swellings.  We  may,  provisionally  at  least,  infer 
from  this  that,  while  considerable  portions  of  both  the  anterior  and 
the  posterior  columns  are  like  the  adjoining  grey  matter  in  some 
way  or  other  concerned  in  the  exit  and  entrance  of  efferent  and 
afferent  fibres,  the  larger  portion  of  the  lateral  column  is  concerned 
in  the  transmission  of  impulses  to  and  fro,  between  the  local 
mechanisms  below,  immediately  connected  with  the  several  spinal 
nerves,  and  the  brain  above.  This  conclusion  seems  incidentally 


CHAP,  i.]  THE   SPINAL   CORD.  887 

confirmed,  (though  these  diagrams  must  not  be  strained  to  carry 
detailed  inferences,)  by  the  sudden  increase  of  the  lateral  column 
above  the  lumbar  swelling,  as  if  the  large  mass  of  nervous 
mechanism  for  the  lower  limbs  concentrated  in  this  region 
demanded  a  sudden  increase  in  the  number  of  fibres  connecting 
it  with  the  brain  above. 

This  more  or  less  continuous  increase  of  the  lateral  column 
partly  explains  the  change  of  form  in  the  general  outline  of  the 
transverse  section  of  the  cord  which  is  observed  in  passing  upwards 
from  the  lower  to  the  higher  regions.  In  the  coccygeal,  sacral  and 
lumbar  regions  the  outline,  though  varying  somewhat  chiefly  owing 
to  the  disposition  of  the  grey  matter,  is  on  the  whole  circular.  In 
the  thoracic  region  especially  in  the  upper  part  the  increase  of  the 
lateral  columns  increases  the  side  to  side  diameter  so  much  that 
the  section  becomes  oval,  and  in  the  cervical  region  this  increase 
of  the  side  to  side  diameter  out  of  proportion  to  the  dorso-ventral 
diameter  is  very  marked.  The  actual  outline  of  the  whole 
transverse  section  is  however  determined  also  to  a  certain  extent 
by  the  changes  of  form  of  the  grey  matter. 

The  cord  moreover  undergoes  along  its  length  a  change  which 
is  not  very  clearly  indicated  in  the  diagrams  Figs.  106,  107.  By 
comparing  the  series  of  transverse  sections  given  in  Fig.  104  it 
will  be  seen  that  the  relative  position  of  the  central  canal  shifts 
along  the  length  of  the  cord.  In  the  sacral  and  lumbar  regions 
the  central  canal  is  nearly  at  the  centre  of  the  circle  of  outline, 
and  the  posterior  and  anterior  fissures  are  nearly  of  equal  depth. 
Even  in  the  upper  lumbar  region,  and  still  more  in  the  thoracic 
region,  the  position  of  the  central  canal  is  shifted  nearer  to  the 
ventral  surface  so  that  the  posterior  fissure  becomes  relatively 
longer,  deeper,  than  the  anterior.  This  shifting  goes  on  through 
the  cervical  region  up  to  about  the  level  of  the  2nd  cervical 
nerve,  where  it  is  arrested  by  the  beginning  of  the  changes 
through  which  the  spinal  cord  is  transformed  into  the  far  more 
complicated  bulb. 

This  lengthening  of  the  posterior  fissure  indicates  an  increase 
in  the  dorso-ventral  diameter  of  the  posterior  columns,  and  this, 
not  being  accompanied  by  a  compensating  diminution  of  the  side 
to  side  diameter,  shews  in  turn  that  the  posterior  columns  undergo 
an  increase  in  passing  upwards.  From  this  we  may  add  to  the 
provisional  conclusion  just  arrived  at  with  regard  to  the  lateral 
columns,  the  further  conclusion  that  some  part  of  the  posterior 
columns  also  is  concerned  in  transmitting  impulses,  in  a  more  or 
less  direct  manner,  between  the  various  regions  of  the  cord  below 
and  the  brain  above.  The  anterior  columns  do  not  increase  in  the 
same  marked  manner,  though  over  and  above  the  increase  due  to 
the  lumbar  and  cervical  swellings,  a  continued  increase  may  be 
observed  especially  in  the  upper  cervical  region ;  it  is  in  this 
upper  region  that  the  direct  pyramidal  tract  is  best  developed. 


888     THE  FEATURES  OF  DIFFERENT  REGIONS.     [BOOK  in. 

§  575.  The  provisional  conclusions  at  which  we  have  arrived 
are  further,  to  a  certain  extent  at  least,  confirmed  and  extended  by 
a  study  of  the  behaviour  at  the  several  regions  of  the  cord  of  the 
special  tracts  of  white  matter  described  in  §  567. 

The  pyramidal  tract,  that  is  to  say,  the  crossed  pyramidal 
tract  entering  the  spinal  cord  above  from  the  pyramid  is  very 
large  in  the  cervical  region,  having  the  form  and  situation  shewn 
in  Fig.  104,  C2C5C8.  From  thence  downward  it  diminishes  in  size, 
the  diminution  being  especially  rapid  in  the  lumbar  swelling, 
Fig.  104,  L1}  where  the  tract  being  no  longer  covered  in  by  the 
cerebellar  tract  comes  to  the  surface  of  the  cord ;  but  it  may  be 
traced  by  the  degeneration  method  down  as  far  as  the  coccygeal 
region,  and  indeed  appears  to  be  coexistent  with  the  entrance 
of  spinal  nerves  into  the  cord.  Diminution  of  the  tract  means  a 
lessening  of  the  number  of  fibres;  and  since  we  cannot  suppose 
that  any  of  the  fibres  come  suddenly  to  an  end  in  the  tract  itself 
we  are  led  to  infer  that  along  the  cord,  from  above  downwards, 
fibres  are  successively  leaving  the  tract  and  passing  to  some  other 
part  of  the  cord.  We  seem  further  justified  in  concluding  that 
the  fibres  which  thus  successively  leave  the  tract  go  to  join  the 
series  of  local  nervous  mechanisms  with  which  the  spinal  nerves 
communicate,  as  we  have  seen  reason  to  believe,  upon  their 
entrance  into  the  cord.  Indeed,  as  we  shall  see  later  on,  we  have 
reason  to  think  that  the  nervous  mechanisms  which  the  fibres  in 
question  join  are  those  belonging  to  the  motor  fibres  of  the 
anterior  roots.  This  pyramidal  tract  does  not  begin  in  the 
pyramid,  but  may  be  traced  through  the  lower  parts  of  the  brain 
right  up  to  special  areas  in  the  cortex  or  surface  of  the  cerebral 
hemispheres ;  and  very  strong  reasons  may  be  brought  forward  in 
support  of  the  view  that  the  fibres  of  this  tract  are  fibres  which 
carry  impulses  from  the  cortex  to  successive  portions  of  the  spinal 
cord,  and  there  give  rise  to  efferent  impulses  which  pass  to 
appropriate  skeletal  muscles.  The  tract,  therefore,  is  not  only  a 
descending  tract  by  virtue  of  the  mode  of  degeneration,  but  may 
be  spoken  of  in  a  broad  sense  as  a  tract  of  efferent  impulses 
descending  from  the  cerebral  cortex ;  and  indeed  it  is  maintained 
that  it  is  the  channel  of  the  particular  kind  of  efferent  impulses 
which  we  shall  speak  of  as  voluntary  or  volitional  impulses.  We 
may  add  that  as  the  tract  passes  along  a  path  which  we  shall 
subsequently  describe,  from  the  cerebral  cortex  through  the  lower 
parts  of  the  brain  to  the  pyramid,  it  gives  off  fibres  to  mechanisms 
connected  with  several  of  the  cranial  nerves,  much  in  the  same 
way  that  it  gives  off  fibres  to  the  spinal  nerves. 

We  may  therefore  picture  to  ourselves  this  pyramidal  tract  as 
starting  in  the  form  of  a  broad  sheaf  of  fibres  from  a  certain 
district  on  the  surface  of  one  of  the  cerebral  hemispheres. 
Putting  aside  for  the  present  any  possible  increase  of  the  number 
of  fibres  by  division  of  fibres  (though  we  have  reason  to  think  that 


CHAP,  i.]  THE   SPINAL   CORD.  889 

this  does  to  a  certain  extent  occur)  we  may  regard  the  tract  as 
being  at  its  maximum  at  its  beginning  in  the  cortex.  As  it 
descends  to  the  decussation  of  the  pyramids  in  the  bulb  it  loses 
a  certain  number  of  fibres,  which  pass  off  to  the  cranial  nerves. 
Having  crossed  and  entered  into  the  lateral  column  of  the  cord  it 
continues  to  give  off  fibres  to  the  spinal  nerves,  probably  to 
the  anterior  root  of  each  in  succession,  and  so  goes  on  its  way 
down  the  cord  continually  diminishing  until  the  last  remaining 
fibres  are  given  off  to  the  last  coccygeal  nerve. 

When  degeneration  is  set  up  along  this  tract,  as  may  be  done, 
by  injuries  to  particular  areas  of  the  cerebral  cortex,  the  main 
mass  of  degenerated  fibres,  after  crossing  over  from  one  side  of 
the  cerebrospinal  axis  to  the  other  in  the  decussation  of  the 
pyramids  at  the  lower  end  of  the  bulb,  during  its  further  progress 
down  the  spinal  cord,  keeps  to  the  side  to  which  it  has  crossed 
right  down  to  the  end.  Hence,  as  we  have  said,  it  is  called  the 
crossed  pyramidal  tract.  The  main  mass  of  fibres,  the  degene- 
ration of  which  has  been  started  by  injury  to  the  left  side  of  the 
brain,  crosses  over  to  the  right  side  of  the  spinal  cord  and  runs 
down  the  lateral  column  of  the  right  side  to  the  end  of  the  cord. 
Nevertheless  some  fibres  appear  to  cross  over  again  in  the  spinal 
cord  and  then  to  run  along  the  same  side  as  the  side  of  the  brain 
injured;  along  the  left  side  in  the  case  just  mentioned.  Such 
fibres  are  spoken  of  as  "recrossed  fibres." 

The  direct  pyramidal  tract  (Fig.  104,  dP),  except  that  it  does 
not  cross  at  the  decussation  of  the  pyramids,  is  otherwise  similar 
to  the  crossed  pyramidal  tract,  and  indeed  is  a  part  of  the  same 
strand  to  which  the  crossed  tract  belongs.  When  degeneration  in 
this  tract  is  started  by  injury  to  particular  areas  of  the  cerebral 
cortex,  say  on  the  left  half  of  the  brain,  the  degeneration  may 
be  traced  through  the  left  anterior  pyramid,  and  so  to  the  left 
median  anterior  column  of  the  spinal  cord.  The  direct  tract  is 
never  so  extensive  or  marked  as  the  crossed  tract,  does  not  reach 
so  far  down,  is  much  more  variable  both  in  length  and  in  sectional 
area  and,  as  we  have  said,  is  almost  confined  to  man.  Diminishing 
as  it  descends  it  may  be  said  to  cease  in  the  middle  thoracic 
region  Fig.  104,  D6D8.  Taking  an  average  we  may  say  that,  of  the 
whole  strand  running  in  the  pyramids  above  the  decussation,  about 
three-fourths  of  the  fibres  go  to  form  the  crossed  and  about  one- 
fourth  to  form  the  direct  tract.  We  shall  see  later  on  that  the 
impulses  coming  down  along  the  united  tract  in  the  brain  may, 
broadly  speaking,  be  said  to  cross  over  wholly  from  one  side  to  the 
other  before  they  reach  the  skeletal  muscles,  so  that  the  impulses 
passing  along  fibres  in,  say,  the  left  pyramid,  reach  the  muscles 
of  the  right  limbs  and  right  side  of  the  body  whether  the  fibres 
cross  over  at  the  decussation  to  form  the  crossed  or  remain  on  the 
same  side  to  form  the  direct  pyramidal  tract.  We  are  therefore 
led  to  infer  that  the  fibres  in  the  direct  tract,  as  they  pass  down  the 


890    THE  FEATURES  OF  DIFFERENT  REGIONS.     [BOOK  in. 

cord,  cross  over  in  the  cord  itself  before  they  make  connections  with 
the  fibres  of  the  anterior  roots.  Probably  the  crossing  is  effected 
by  means  of  some  of  the  decussating  fibres  which  form  the 
anterior  white  commissure.  A  part  only,  indeed  a  small  part,  of 
the  commissure  can  serve  this  purpose ;  most  of  the  fibres  of  the 
commissure,  and  in  the  lower  regions  of  the  cord,  where  the  direct 
tract  no  longer  exists,  all  the  fibres  must  have  some  other  functions. 
Some  of  the  fibres  of  this  great  pyramidal  tract,  leave  the  tract, 
as  we  have  said,  to  join  some  of  the  cranial  nerves  before  the 
pyramids  of  the  bulb  are  reached ;  and  the  impulses  passing 
along  these  fibres  also  cross  over  to  the  opposite  side  before  they 
issue  along  the  cranial  nerves.  Hence  we  infer  that  these  fibres 
decussate  above  the  decussation  of  the  pyramids  just  as  those  of 
the  direct  tract  decussate  below  it.  So  that  of  the  whole  strand 
as  it  leaves  the  cerebral  cortex,  while  the  main  mass  of  fibres 
crosses  over  at  the  decussation  of  the  pyramids,  the  rest  of  the 
fibres  cross  the  middle  line  in  succession  from  the  level  of  the 
third  cranial  nerve  to  the  level  of  the  lower  limit  of  the  direct 
tract ;  below  the  decussation  of  the  pyramids  the  crossing  takes 
place  by  means  of  the  anterior  commissure  of  the  cord,  above  the 
decussation  by  means  of  what  we  shall  later  on  learn  to  speak  of 
as  the  raphe  of  the  bulb,  or  by  structures  corresponding  to  this 
higher  up. 

§  576.  The  cerebellar  tract  (Fig.  104,  Cb)  is  as  we  have  seen  a 
tract  of  ascending  degeneration ;  the  degeneration  in  it  makes  its 
appearance  above  the  section  or  the  seat  of  other  injury  of  the 
cord.  It  begins  somewhat  suddenly  at  the  level  of  the  second 
lumbar  nerve  region,  being  absent  at  least  as  a  distinct  tract 
below ;  injury  of  the  cord  at  the  level  of  the  middle  and  lower 
lumbar  nerves  leads  to  no  marked  tract  of  degeneration  (though 
possibly  scattered  single  fibres  may  degenerate),  while  injury 
higher  up  does.  The  tract  lies,  as  we  have  said,  close  to  the 
surface  of  the  cord  in  the  posterior  part  of  the  lateral  column  just 
outside  the  crossed  pyramidal  tract,  and  while  varying  somewhat 
in  the  shape  of  its  section  from  level  to  level  remains  throughout 
a  somewhat  narrow  crescentic  patch.  At  the  top  of  the  spinal 
cord  it  passes,  as  we  have  said,  from  the  lateral  columns  into  the 
restiform  bodies  of  the  bulb,  and  so  to  certain  parts  of  the 
cerebellum. 

When  the  section  or  lesion  is  limited  to  one  side  of  the  cord, 
the  degeneration  is  similarly  limited  to  the  same  side,  and  that 
along  its  whole  course  up  to  the  cerebellum ;  there  is  no  evidence 
of  any  of  the  fibres  decussating  in  the  cord. 

The  area  of  the  tract  increases  from  below  upward.  This  has 
been  determined  by  the  embryological  method,  by  noting  the 
appearance  of  the  medulla  in  the  fibres,  as  well  as  by  comparing 
the  extent  of  the  degeneration  following  upon  a  section  high  up 
in  the  cord  with  that  following  upon  a  section  lower  down.  From 


CHAP.  L]  THE   SPINAL   CORD.  891 

this  we  infer  that  the  fibres  composing  the  tract  must  start 
successively  from  other  parts  of  the  cord  along  its  length,  that  is 
to  say,  the  tract  must  be  fed  by  fibres  coming  from  other  structures 
in  the  cord.  On  the  other  hand,  it  is  found  that  the  degenerated 
area  following  upon  a  section  or  injury  diminishes  as  it  is  traced 
upward ;  when,  for  instance,  a  section  is  made  in  the  mid  thoracic 
region  the  area  of  degeneration  in  the  tract  is  greater  immediately 
above  the  section  than  it  is  higher  up,  say  in  the  cervical  region. 
From  this  we  are  led  to  infer  that  though  the  tract  is  successively 
fed  along  its  course  by  fibres  coming  from  other  parts  of  the 
cord,  some  of  the  fibres  entering  the  tract,  though  like  their 
companions  undergoing  an  ascending  degeneration,  do  not  like 
them  continue  in  the  tract  right  up  to  the  cerebellum,  but  pass  off 
to  other  parts  of  the  cord  on  their  way  upward.  This,  however,  is 
equivalent  to  saying  that  the  tract  is  not  a  pure  or  homogene- 
ous one,  but  consists  of  at  least  two  sets  of  fibres,  only  one  of 
which  is  continued  on  to  the  cerebellum  and  strictly  deserves  the 
name  of  '  cerebellar.'  It  may  perhaps  here  be  mentioned  that 
while  the  fibres  composing  the  tract  are  as  a  whole  conspicuously 
coarse,  large  fibres,  with  these  there  are  mingled,  especially  in 
the  thoracic  region,  a  number  of  much  finer  fibres ;  but  these 
apparently  undergo  a  descending  not  an  ascending  degeneration 
and  do  not  therefore  really  belong  to  the  tract ;  they  may  be 
fibres  which  have  strayed  from  the  pyramidal  tract. 

We  have  as  yet  no  very  clear  evidence  as  to  the  origin  of  the 
fibres  which  compose  the  tract.  Unlike  the  case  of  the  median 
posterior  tract  of  which  we  have  next  to  speak,  no  degeneration, 
at  least  in  the  lumbar  and  thoracic  regions,  appears  in  the  tract 
after  section  merely  of  the  roots  of  the  nerves ;  to  produce  the 
degeneration  the  cord  itself  must  be  injured.  From  this  we  may 
infer  that  the  tract  is  not  fed  directly  by  the  fibres  of  the  posterior 
roots.  Some  observers  maintain  that  the  tract  is  fed  by  fibres 
coming  from  the  vesicular  cylinder  and  point  out  that  both  the 
tract  and  the  column  begin  at  the  same  level  somewhat  suddenly ; 
but  the  want  of  parallelism  between  the  course  of  the  tract  and 
that  of  the  cylinder  along  the  length  of  the  cord,  the  latter  being 
as  we  said  conspicuous  in  the  thoracic  region  while  the  tract 
steadily  increases  upward,  is  distinctly  opposed  to  such  a  view. 
From  the  fact  that  the  degeneration  taking  place  in  it  is  an 
ascending  one,  it  is  supposed  that  the  tract  is  the  channel  for 
ascending,  that  is  to  say,  in  a  broad  sense,  afferent  impulses.  And 
considerable  interest  attaches  to  the  fact  that  these  impulses 
should  be  carried,  not  to  the  cerebrum  but  to  the  cerebellum. 
Our  knowledge  on  this  point,  however,  is  very  imperfect,  and 
what  can  be  said  in  the  matter  had  better  be  said  later  on. 

§  577.  The  median  posterior  tract  is  the  other  conspicuous 
tract  of  ascending  degeneration;  it  also  is  supposed  to  be  a 
channel  for  ascending,  afferent  impulses ;  and  this  view  is  rendered 


892     THE  FEATURES  OF  DIFFERENT  REGIONS.    [BOOK  in. 

almost  certain  by  the  intimate  relations  of  the  tract  to  the  fibres 
of  the  posterior  roots. 

In  dealing  so  far  with  the  tracts  of  degeneration  in  the  spinal 
cord  we  have  always  spoken  of  the  degeneration  as  being  the 
result  of  lesions  of  the  spinal  cord  itself.  Experiments  on  animals, 
however,  and  clinical  experience  have  shewn  that  division  or 
injury  of  the  fibres  of  the  posterior  roots  is  followed  by  tracts 
of  degeneration  in  the  spinal  cord,  though  no  damage  whatever 
may  have  been  done  to  the  substance  of  the  cord  itself.  These 
tracts  make  their  appearance  in  the  median  posterior  columns,  the 
exact  path  and  limits  of  the  degeneration  differing  with  the 
different  spinal  nerves.  The  results  of  the  division  of  different 
groups  of  nerves  are  so  instructive  that  we  may  dwell  upon  them 
in  detail. 

If  the  posterior  roots  of  two  or  three  lumbar  nerves  (on  one 
side),  be  divided,  an  examination  of  the  cord,  after  an  interval 
long  enough  to  allow  degeneration  to  be  well  established,  will 
bring  to  light  the  following  features.  The  divided  roots  will  be 
found  to  have  degenerated  right  up  to  their  entrance  into  the 
cord.  A  section  of  the  cord  opposite  the  entrance  of  the  lowest 
divided  root  will  shew  no  degeneration  of  the  cord  beyond 
that  of  the  bundles  of  fibres  passing  in.  A  little  higher  up 
degeneration  will  be  observed  in  the  external  posterior  column 
close  to  the  posterior  horn;  and  as  we  ascend  we  find  that  this 
degeneration  first  spreads  over  a  large  portion  of  the  external 
posterior  column,  and  then  invades  the  median  posterior  column ; 
the  degeneration  does  not  affect  the  whole  of  the  median  posterior 
column  but  leaves  intact  a  small  dorsal  portion,  roughly  tri- 
angular in  shape,  at  the  angle  between  the  fissure  and  the 
dorsal  surface  of  the  cord,  as  well  as  some  portion  of  the  more 
ventral  part  of  the  column  nearest  the  grey  commissure.  Still 
a  little  higher  up  we  should  find  that  degenerated  fibres  had 
disappeared  from  the  external  portion  of  the  external  posterior 
column  close  to  the  grey  matter,  though  still  existing  in  the  more 
median  part  of  that  column,  as  well  as  in  the  median  posterior 
column  to  the  extent  just  indicated.  Still  a  little  higher  up 
the  whole  of  the  degeneration  would  have  disappeared  from  the 
external  posterior  column,  but  the  tract  of  degeneration  in  the 
median  posterior  column  would  remain,  the  extent  of  degeneration 
being  dependent  on  the  number  of  roots  which  had  been  divided. 
Lastly,  by  carrying  the  sections  still  higher  up  the  cord  we  should 
be  able  to  trace  this  tract  in  the  median  posterior  column  right 
up  to  the  bulb,  where  it  would  come  to  an  end. 

If  we  divided  some  of  the  thoracic  nerves  instead  of  the 
lumbar  we  should  obtain  very  similar  results:  a  degeneration  of 
the  external  posterior  columns  a  little  above  the  entrance  of  the 
roots,  spreading  across  the  column  towards  the  median  line,  and 
wholly  disappearing  at  a  certain  height  above,  accompanied  by  a 


CHAP.  L]  THE   SPINAL   CORD.  893 

degeneration  of  a  part  of  the  median  posterior  column,  reaching 
from  a  little  distance  above  the  entrance  of  the  divided  nerve-roots 
right  up  to  the  bulb.  This  latter  tract  of  degeneration  would 
however  not  occupy  the  same  position  as  that  consequent  upon 
division  of  the  lumbar  nerves ;  its  position  would  be  more  ventral, 
nearer  the  grey  commissure,  and  rather  more  lateral.  Compare 
Fig.  104,  D2,  where  IT  indicates  the  degeneration  due  to  section 
of  the  lumbar  nerves,  and  dr  that  of  the  thoracic  nerves.  If  we 
divided  some  of  the  cervical  posterior  roots  we  should  get  similar 
results,  with  the  difference  that  the  tract  of  degeneration  in  the 
median  posterior  columns  would  occupy  a  position  still  more 
ventral  and  still  more  lateral  (Fig.  104,  C5  c.r.) ;  while  if  we 
divided  the  sacral  nerves  the  tract  of  degeneration  would  be  dorsal 
and  median  to  the  tract  belonging  to  the  lumbar  nerves,  and 
would  occupy  more  or  less  of  the  triangle  left  below  that  tract 
(Fig.  104,  D2s.r.).  The  degeneration  it  will  be  understood  is  in  all 
cases  confined  to  the  same  side  of  the  cord  as  that  of  the  divided 
roots.  We  may  add,  in  order  to  complete  the  story  of  the  effects 
of  division  of  the  posterior  roots,  that  the  section  leads  to 
degeneration  of  the  marginal  zone  (Lissauer's  tract),  but  this 
degeneration  reaches  for  a  certain  distance  only  up  the  cord  and 
then  disappears.  It  will  be  remembered  that  this  zone  is  fed  by 
fibres  (of  fine  calibre)  belonging  to  the  external  or  lateral  bundle 
of  the  posterior  roots. 

These  results  may  be  interpreted  as  follows.  The  (great 
majority  of  the)  fibres  of  the  posterior  root,  cut  off  from  their 
ganglion  by  the  division,  degenerate  centripetally  towards  the 
spinal  cord.  We  have  previously  seen  that  many  of  the  fibres 
of  the  root  pass  into  the  external  posterior  column  and  run  up  in 
that  column  for  some  distance.  The  degeneration  observed  in 
this  column  for  some  distance  above  the  entrance  of  the  divided 
roots  shews  that  the  fibres  run  lengthways  for  some  distance  in 
this  column,  while  the  disappearance  of  the  degeneration  a  little 
higher  up  similarly  shews  that  the  fibres  eventually  leave  the 
column.  The  appearance  of  degeneration  in  the  median  posterior 
column  shews  that  some  of  these  fibres  have  passed  into  that 
column  from  the  external  posterior  column,  and  the  continuation 
of  that  degeneration  right  up  to  the  bulb  indicates  that  these 
fibres  pursue  an  unbroken  course  in  that  column  along  the 
whole  length  of  the  cord.  The  area  of  degeneration,  or  more 
exactly  the  number  of  degenerated  fibres  in  the  continued  tract 
of  degeneration  in  the  median  posterior  column  is  much  less 
than  that  in  the  temporary  or  short  tract  of  degeneration  in  the 
external  posterior  column.  This  shews  that  some  only  of  the 
fibres  passing  into  the  external  posterior  column  go  on  to  join  the 
median  posterior  column  and  so  reach  the  bulb ;  the  rest  obviously 
take  another  path,  and  we  have  already  seen  reason  to  think  that 
many  of  these  end  in  the  grey  matter  of  the  cord.  Hence  of  all 


894    THE  FEATURES  OF  DIFFERENT  REGIONS.     [BOOK  m. 

the  fibres  joining  the  cord  in  a  posterior  root,  while  some,  and  these 
we  may  add  are  chiefly  fine  fibres,  entering  the  grey  matter 
directly  or  passing  into  the  posterior  marginal  zone,  soon  make 
such  connections  that  the  degeneration  due  to  the  section  of  the 
root  spreads  no  farther,  a  large  number,  and  these  chiefly  coarse 
fibres,  before  they  make  any  such  connection  pass  into  and 
occupy  for  some  length  of  the  cord  the  external  posterior 
column.  We  may  here  remark  that  though  these  fibres  are 
spread  over  the  greater  part  of  this  column,  they  do  not  form 
the  whole  of  the  column;  they  are  mixed  up  with  fibres  of  a 
different  nature  and  origin.  Of  these  fibres  of  the  posterior  root 
which  thus  run  in  the  external  posterior  column  while  still 
dependent  for  their  nutritive  activity  on  the  ganglion  of  the 
root,  some,  indeed  the  greater  part,  leave  the  tract  and  make 
such  connections  in  the  grey  matter,  that  their  degeneration 
ceases;  others,  forming  the  smaller  part,  pass  into  the  median 
posterior  column,  and  taking  up  a  definite  position  in  that  column 
pursue  an  unbroken  course  to  the  bulb. 

All  the  fibres  therefore  of  the  posterior  roots  do  not  end  in 
the  grey  matter  soon  after  their  entrance  into  the  cord.  A  repre- 
sentative of  each  root  is  carried  right  up  to  the  bulb  by  means 
of  the  median  posterior  column ;  of  the  axis-cylinders  which  leave 
the  ganglion  on  the  root,  a  certain  relatively  small  number  pursue 
an  unbroken  course  for  some  little  distance  through  the  external 
posterior  column,  and  for  the  rest  of  their  way  through  the 
median  posterior  column,  along  the  whole  length  of  the  cord  above 
the  entrance  of  the  root  until  they  find  an  ending  in  the  grey 
matter  of  the  bulb.  Further,  each  spinal  nerve  has  this  represent- 
ative of  its  posterior  root  placed  in  a  definite  position  in  the 
posterior  median  column,  the  arrangement  being  such  as  shewn 
in  Fig.  104,  that  the  lower  (sacral)  nerves  find  their  place  in  the 
more  dorsal  and  median  part  of  the  column,  while  the  nerves 
above  are  successively  placed  in  positions  more  and  more  ventral 
and  external. 

As  far  as  our  knowledge  goes  at  present  we  are  led  to  believe 
that  this  median  posterior  tract  is  very  largely  made  up  of  fibres 
having  this  origin.  It  affords  a  channel  by  which  afferent  impulses 
are  carried  straight  up  the  cord  from  the  nerve  trunk  without 
making  connections  on  the  way.  We  may  repeat  that  the  path  is 
confined  to  the  same  side  of  the  cord  along  its  whole  length ; 
there  is  no  crossing  over  to  the  other  side. 

In  the  above  description  we  have  spoken  only  of  the  results 
following  section  of  the  posterior  roots  outside  the  cord;  but  it 
will  be  understood  that  similar  results  follow  upon  section  of  or 
injury  to  or  disease  of  the  cord  itself  affecting  the  posterior 
columns  or  the  bundles  of  the  roots  as  they  enter  the  cord. 
When  such  a  lesion  occurs  there  may  be  observed  in  the  region 
of  the  cord  above  the  lesion  a  degeneration  of  the  external 


CHAP,  i.]  THE   SPINAL  CORD.  895 

posterior  column,  reaching  some  little  distance  up,  and  a  more 
limited  degeneration  of  a  part  of  the  median  posterior  column 
stretching  right  up  to  the  bulb.  The  position  and  form  of  the 
tract  of  the  degeneration  in  the  median  posterior  column  will 
depend  on  the  level  of  the  lesion  along  the  length  of  the  cord, 
according  as  it  interrupts  the  ascending  representatives  of  the 
sacral  nerves  only,  or  of  the  lumbar  and  sacral  nerves,  or  of  the 
dorsal  and  cervical  nerves  as  well.  A  complete  section  or  hemi- 
section  of  the  cord  will  produce  results  corresponding  to  the  division 
on  both  sides  or  on  one  side  of  all  the  nerves  below  the  section. 

We  may  add  that  while,  according  to  some  observers,  the 
strand  of  fibres  belonging  to  a  particular  root  or  group  of  roots, 
having  once  taken  up  its  position  in  the  median  posterior  column 
remains  unchanged  until  it  reaches  the  bulb ;  according  to  others 
it  diminishes  in  area,  some  of  its  fibres  making  connections  in  the 
cord  itself. 

§  578.  The  antero-lateral  ascending  tract  (Fig.  104,  asc.  a.  I.) 
is  less  well  known  than  either  of  the  two  preceding;  it  is  also 
more  diffuse,  that  is  to  say,  the  fibres  undergoing  degeneration  are 
more  largely  mixed  with  fibres  of  a  different  nature  and  origin. 
It  appears  to  extend  down  the  cord  to  a  lower  level  than  the 
cerebellar  tract,  but  its  lower  limit  has  not  yet  been  accurately 
determined.  Since  the  degeneration  taking  place  in  it  is  an 
ascending  one,  it  has  been  inferred  that  it  serves  as  the  path 
for  afferent,  and  indeed  for  sensory  impulses.  Degeneration  in 
it  is  seen  only  after  section  or  injury  of  the  substance  of  the  cord 
itself,  not  after  division  of  the  posterior  roots.  If,  then,  it  is  to 
be  regarded  as  a  channel  of  afferent  impulses  passing  into  it 
from  the  posterior  roots,  those  impulses  must  pass  into  it  along 
those  fibres  of  the  posterior  root  which  find  secondary  trophic 
centres  in  some  part  of  the  grey  matter ;  in  this  respect  this  tract 
resembles  the  cerebellar  tract,  and  differs  from  the  median 
posterior  tract.  The  latter  is  the  direct  continuation  up  the  cord 
to  the  bulb  of  such  fibres  as  are  still  trusting  for  their  nutritive 
activity  to  the  cells  of  the  ganglion  on  the  posterior  root ;  the 
fibres  of  both  the  former  trust  for  their  nutritive  activity  to  some 
part  of  the  grey  matter  of  the  cord,  and  presumably  to  the  nerve- 
cells  of  that  grey  matter.  A  further  resemblance  between  the 
antero-lateral  ascending  and  cerebellar  tracts  must  be  admitted,  if 
future  researches  confirm  the  opinion  of  those  who  hold  that  the 
former  like  the  latter,  at  the  top  of  the  cord,  pass  along  the 
restiform  body  to  the  cerebellum.  Indeed  under  such  a  view  it 
would  appear  probable  that  the  antero-lateral  tract  is  simply  a 
more  diffuse  and  outlying  part  of  the  cerebellar  tract. 

§  579.  We  may  now  briefly  pass  in  review,  somewhat  as 
follows,  the  chief  facts  which  we  have  learnt  concerning  the 
structure  of  the  spinal  cord,  always  keeping  in  view  their  physio- 
logical meaning. 

F.  57 


896       THE   NATURE   OF   THE   GREY   MATTER.     [BOOK  m. 

The  important  feature  of  the  spinal  cord  is  the  presence  of 
what  we  have  called  'grey  matter/  and  all  our  knowledge  goes  to 
shew  that  the  important  powers  of  the  spinal  cord,  by  which  it 
differs  from  a  thick  multiple  nerve,  and  by  virtue  of  which  we 
speak  of  it  as  a  nervous  centre  or  series  of  centres,  are  in  some  way 
or  other  associated  with  this  grey  matter. 

With  this  grey  matter  the  fibres  of  the  spinal  nerves  are 
connected.  The  greater  part  of  the  fibres  of  the  anterior  root 
certainly  end  in  or  rather  take  origin  from  the  grey  matter  close 
to  the  attachment  of  the  root,  and  the  rest  most  probably  join 
the  grey  matter  at  no  great  distance.  The  fibres  of  the  posterior 
root  run,  as  we  have  seen,  for  some  little  distance  in  the  white 
matter,  but,  if  we  except  the  special  bundle  which  runs  in  the 
median  posterior  tract  right  up  the  cord  to  the  bulb  without 
joining  the  spinal  grey  matter  at  all,  we  may  say  that  the  fibres 
of  the  posterior  root  also  join  the  grey  matter  not  far  from  the 
attachment  of  the  root. 

Morphological  reasons  lead  us,  as  we  have  seen,  to  regard  the 
spinal  cord  as  a  series  of  segments,  each  segment  corresponding 
to  a  pair  of  nerves ;  and  even  in  the  spinal  cord  of  man  we  may 
recognize  a  segmental  groundwork,  obscured  though  this  is  by 
fusion  and  overlaid  by  the  several  commissural  tracts.  Each 
segment  of  this  groundwork  we  may  conceive  of  as  a  central  mass 
of  grey  matter,  connected  on  each  side  with  an  anterior  and  a 
posterior  root,  thus  constituting  a  segmental  nervous  mechanism 
capable  of  carrying  out  certain  functions. 

Such  a  segment  has  been  compared  to  a  ganglion,  but  it  differs 
strikingly  from  a  ganglion,  whether  of  the  posterior  root  or  of  the 
splanchnic  system,  both  in  structure  and  in  function.  A  ganglion 
and  the  grey  matter  of  a  spinal  segment  both  contain  nerve-cells, 
and  so  far  resemble  each  other;  but  there  the  resemblance  for 
the  most  part  ends.  In  a  ganglion  the  constituent  nerve-cell  is  a 
development  of  the  axis-cylinder  of  a  fibre  into  a  nucleated  cell- 
body  which  lies  on  the  course  of  the  fibre,  and  may,  as  in  a 
splanchnic  ganglion,  be  placed  just  where  one  fibre  divides  into 
two  or  more.  We  have  clear  evidence  that  the  cell,  that  is  to  say, 
the  nucleus  with  the  adjacent  cell-substance,  exercises  an  important 
influence  on  the  nutrition,  and  so  on  the  functional  activity  of  the 
nerve-fibre ;  it  acts  as  we  have  seen  as  a  '  trophic  centre.'  There 
are  also  reasons  for  thinking  that  the  cell-substance  is  more 
sensitive,  more  readily  responsive  to  changes  in  its  circumstances 
than  is  the  axis-cylinder  at  some  distance  from  the  cell.  But  we 
have  no  satisfactory  evidence  that  the  cell  can  automatically 
originate  nervous  impulses  in  itself,  as  the  outcome  of  its  own 
intrinsic  changes.  Nor  have  we  any  evidence  that  the  cell  can 
exert  any  marked  transforming  power  over  the  impulses  passing 
along  the  fibre ;  the  impulses  which  travel  away  from  the  cell  do 
not  appear  to  differ  markedly  from  those  which  travel  towards  it. 


CHAP,  i.]  THE   SPINAL   CORD.  897 

The  several  instances  in  which  there  seemed  to  be  evidence  that 
splanchnic  ganglia  acted  as  centres  either  of  reflex  or  of  automatic 
action,  have  as  we  have  seen  broken  down ;  and  it  is  not  even 
suggested  that  the  ganglia  of  the  posterior  roots  possess  any  such 
powers.  The  grey  matter  of  the  spinal  cord,  on  the  other  hand, 
as  we  have  already  seen,  and  as  we  shall  see  more  in  detail,  is 
especially  characterized  by  the  possession  of  reflex  and  automatic 
as  well  as  of  other  powers. 

In  structure,  moreover,  such  a  spinal  segment  differs  strikingly 
from  a  ganglion  and  exhibits  features  unknown  in  ganglia. 
In  a  ganglion  the  nerve  fibres  may  divide,  and  in  a  small 
peripheral  ganglion  the  division  may  give  rise  to  very  delicate 
fibrils ;  but  the  fibres  or  fibrils  resulting  from  the  division  leave 
the  ganglion  to  follow  their  appropriate  courses ;  the  division  serves 
for  dispersion  only.  In  the  spinal  cord  on  the  other  hand  both 
efferent  and  afferent  fibres  divide  in  such  a  way  that  their 
divisions  are  lost  to  view  in  the  grey  matter ;  division  here  seems 
to  serve  the  purpose  of  union.  The  efferent  fibre  of  the  anterior 
root  may  be  traced  back  as  a  process  of  a  cell  in  the  anterior  horn. 
That  cell  gives  off  other  processes,  but  no  one  of  these  processes  is 
continued  on  as  an  axis-cylinder  process  stretching  across  the 
grey  matter  until  it  becomes  a  fibre  of  the  posterior  root,  or  as 
anything  like  such  an  axis-cylinder  process.  On  the  contrary,  all 
the  processes,  except  the  axis-cylinder  process,  divide  into  branches, 
and  appear  to  end  in  nervous  fibrils  lost  to  view  in  the  grey 
matter.  Conversely,  though  our  knowledge  of  the  junction  of  the 
posterior  fibres  with  the  grey  matter  is  much  more  imperfect  than 
that  of  the  junction  of  the  anterior  fibres,  what  we  do  know  leads 
us  to  believe  that  the  fibres  of  the  posterior  root,  either  by  the 
mediation  of  cells,  or  by  -direct  division  of  the  axis-cylinder 
without  the  mediation  of  cells,  similarly  break  up  into  fibrils  and 
are  similarly  lost  in  the  grey  matter.  All  the  evidence  goes 
to  shew  that  the  anterior  and  posterior  roots  are  functionally 
continuous  ;  this  functional  continuity  is,  however,  effected  not  by 
a  gross  continuity  of  axis-cylinders  but  in  a  peculiar  manner 
through  the  division  of  branches  of  nerve-cells  or  of  axis-cylinders 
into  the  nervous  tangle  which  forms  such  a  special  feature  of  the 
grey  matter  of  the  cord.  We  may  perhaps  venture  to  regard  the 
grey  matter  of  the  segmental  groundwork,  of  which  we  are  now 
alone  speaking,  as  constituting  a  nervous  network  or  web,  formed 
certainly  in  part  by  the  rapidly  dividing  branches  of  nerve-cells, 
and  probably  in  part  by  the  divisions  of  directly  dividing  nerve 
fibres. 

In  any  ordinary  section  of  the  spinal  cord  the  grey  matter 
presents  to  view  much  more  than  this  nervous  groundwork. 
To  say  nothing  of  the  indubitable  neuroglia  and  the  obscure 
structures,  including  small  cells,  which  are  claimed  now  to  be 
neuroglia,  now  to  be  nervous  in  nature,  the  grey  matter  in  every 

57—2 


898        THE   NATURE   OF  THE   GREY   MATTER.     [BOOK  m. 

section  shews  numerous  distinct  nerve  fibres  crossing  it  in  various 
directions ;  of  these  fibres  a  few  are  ordinary  medullated  fibres, 
some  are  non-medullated  fibres,  that  is  to  say,  are  naked  axis- 
cylinders,  while  others,  and  these  the  more  numerous,  are  the 
peculiar  medullated  fibres  of  small  diameter  spoken  of  in  §  563. 
A  large  number  of  these  fibres,  indeed  all  the  larger  ones,  though 
they  go  to  make  up  what  we  call  grey  matter,  are  not  continuous 
with,  and  do  not  belong  to,  the  groundwork  or  nervous  web,  at  all 
events,  do  not  form  part  of  the  groundwork  seen  in  the  same 
section  as  themselves.  They  are  simply  fibres  traversing  the 
groundwork,  in  spaces  of  the  neuroglia  bed,  on  their  way  up  or 
down  the  cord  or  across  the  cord  from  one  part  to  another.  It 
may  be  that  some  of  the  finer  medullated  fibres  do  really  enter 
into  the  groundwork,  and  so  contribute  to  the  nervous  web ;  but 
our  knowledge  is  too  imperfect  to  afford  a  clear  decision  on  this 
point.  Our  inability  to  define  its  exact  limits  need  not,  however, 
prevent  our  recognising  the  existence  of  the  groundwork. 

The  prominence  in  this  groundwork  of  the  larger  nerve-cells 
has  led  to  the  conception  that  the  powers  of  the  spinal  segment 
are  exercised  by  these  nerve-cells  to  the  exclusion  of  the  other 
elements  of  the  nervous  web.  But  such  a  view  has  not  been 
adequately  proved.  What  we  do  know  is  that  the  nuclei  and 
cell-bodies  of  the  cells  of  the  anterior  horn  exercise  an  important 
influence  on  the  nutrition  of  the  fibres  of  the  anterior  root  which 
proceed  from  them,  and  possibly  also  influence  the  nutrition  of  the 
other  branches  of  the  cells  forming  part  of  the  groundwork ;  and 
these  cells  are  probably  so  conspicuous  a  feature  of  every  section 
of  the  spinal  cord  because  of  the  important  task  entrusted  to  them 
of  maintaining  in  due  order  the  nutrition  of  the  long  stretch  of 
motor  fibres  reaching  from  them  to  the  muscular  fibres  or  other 
peripheral  organs.  The  fibres  of  the  posterior  root  are  not  so 
obviously  connected  with  the  conspicuous  cells  of  the  grey 
matter;  indeed  as  we  have  said  it  may  be  doubted,  though  the 
view  is  maintained  by  some,  whether  any  cell  intervenes  to  secure 
the  continuity  of  a  posterior  fibre  with  the  groundwork,  a  division 
of  the  axis-cylinder  serving  this  purpose;  and  this  becomes 
intelligible  when  we  bear  in  mind  that  the  posterior  fibres  are 
governed  as  far  as  their  nutrition  is  concerned  by  the  nerve-cells 
of  the  ganglion  on  the  posterior  root,  which  ought  probably  to  be 
considered  as  much  a  part  of  the  spinal  cord  as  the  cells  of  the 
anterior  horn.  The  nerve-cell  of  the  ganglion  is  adequate  to 
secure  the  due  nutrition  of  the  nerve  fibre  until  it  joins  the 
groundwork,  and  probably  helps  to  maintain  the  nutrition  of  the 
groundwork  itself. 

Hence  we  may  perhaps,  until  fresh  evidence  to  the  contrary  is 
brought  forward,  incline  to  the  view  that  the  powers  of  the  grey 
matter  do  not  depend  on  the  conspicuous  cells  alone  or  even 
chiefly,  but  on  the  peculiar  molecular  constitution  and  nature  of 


CHAP,  i.]  THE   SPINAL   CORD.  899 

the  whole  groundwork.  The  nuclei  of  the  cells  of  the  anterior 
horn  with  the  cell-substance  adjacent  to  each  and  the  cells  of  the 
ganglia  on  the  posterior  root  probably  govern  the  nutrition,  and 
so  the  functional  activity  of  the  groundwork  as  well  as  of  the 
issuing  and  entering  fibres ;  but  there  appears  to  be  as  yet  no 
convincing  evidence  of  any  other  peculiar  powers  confined  to  the 
cells  and  absent  from  other  parts  of  the  groundwork.  We  may 
add  that,  in  accordance  with  this  view,  the  other  cells  of  the  grey 
matter,  such  as  those  of  the  vesicular  cylinder,  are  to  be  regarded 
as  of  importance  for  governing  the  nutrition  of  fibres,  commissural 
and  others,  starting  from  the  spinal  segment,  and  of  the  part  of 
the  groundwork  from  which  by  their  mediation  the  fibres  start, 
rather  than  for  determining  the  functions  of  the  groundwork  of 
the  segment  or  of  the  fibres  receiving  impulses  from  it. 

§  580.  The  segmental  groundwork  of  grey  matter  belonging 
to  each  pair  of  spinal  nerves  is  so  fused  with  that  of  all  the 
other  pairs,  as  to  form  along  the  whole  length  of  the  cord  a  mass 
of  grey  matter  which  appears,  under  certain  circumstances  at  all 
events,  to  be  continuous  in  the  sense  that  impulses  may  pass  in 
all  directions  along  it.  But  each  spinal  segment  is  in  addition 
connected  by  means  of  tracts  of  white  matter  with  parts  more  or 
less  distant.  The  crossed  pyramidal  tract  is  such  a  longitudinal 
commissural  tract,  connecting  apparently  each  spinal  segment  in 
succession  with  a  certain  part  of  the  cortex  of  the  cerebrum.  We 
have  reason  to  think,  as  we  shall  see  later  on,  that  impulses 
descending  this  or  that  fibre  or  group  of  fibres  of  this  tract  give 
rise  to  the  issue  of  motor  impulses  along  this  or  that  fibre  or 
group  of  fibres  of  an  anterior  root.  We  do  not  at  present  know 
what  is  the  exact  manner  by  which  the  fibre  in  the  pyramidal 
tract  is  connected  with  the  fibre  of  the  anterior  root.  It  seems 
certain,  however,  that  the  connection  is  not  in  the  form  of  a  fibre 
isolated  from  the  rest  of  the  grey  matter,  continuing,  so  to  speak, 
the  pyramidal  fibre  into  a  cell  of  the  anterior  horn  whence  the 
fibre  of  the  anterior  root  issues.  Most  probably  the  pyramidal 
fibre  makes  connections  with  the  segmental  groundwork  spoken 
of  above,  whether  with  or  without  the  intervention  of  a  cell  we 
cannot  at  present  tell.  The  direct  pyramidal  tract  is  a  like 
tract  of  less  extent  downwards,  and  the  less  known  antero-lateral 
descending  tract  is  probably  of  a  similar  nature. 

The  cerebellar  and  antero-lateral  ascending  tracts  are  in  like 
manner  to  be  regarded  as  longitudinal  commissures  between  the 
successive  spinal  segments  below  and  some  part  of  the  brain  above. 
We  have  reason  to  think  that  these  tracts  convey  upward  impulses 
of  a  nature  which  may  be  called  afferent,  and  are  therefore  in  some 
way  probably  connected  with  the  posterior  roots.  We  do  not 
know  as  yet  the  exact  nature  of  the  connection ;  but  probably  in 
those  cases  also,  the  commissural  fibres  are  united  not  directly  to 
the  posterior  fibres,  but  indirectly  by  means  of  the  segmental 


900  THE   COMMISSURAL   TRACTS.  [BOOK  in. 

groundwork.  And  since  these  tracts  do  not  degenerate  after 
section  of  the  posterior  roots,  but  only  after  section  or  other  lesion 
of  the  cord  itself,  we  may  infer  that  their  junction  with  the 
groundwork  is  effected  by  means  of  trophic  cells,  by  means  of 
some  or  other  of  the  cells  spoken  of  a  little  while  before. 

The  median  posterior  tract  seems  to  be  a  commissural  tract  of 
a  nature  different  from  any  of  the  above.  Through  it  a  certain 
part  of  each  posterior  root  is  brought  into  connection,  not  with  its 
own  spinal  segment  but  with  the  bulb  above,  and  so  with  the  brain, 
which  thus  receives  direct  representatives  of  each  afferent  spinal 
nerve.  If  however,  as  some  maintain,  the  bundle  in  this  tract 
starting  from  a  spinal  nerve  below,  diminishes  as  it  proceeds 
upwards,  throwing  off  fibres  to  pass  elsewhere,  though  always 
carrying  some  fibres  right  up  to  the  bulb,  we  must  add  to  the 
above  the  further  view  that  this  tract  connects  also  each  posterior 
root,  not  with  its  own  segment  but  with  other  more  or  less 
distant  segments. 

§  581.  All  the  evidence  which  we  possess  goes  to  shew  that 
each  strand  of  each  of  these  tracts  runs  isolated,  that  is  to  say, 
makes  no  connections  with  adjoining  structures  at  any  part  of 
its  course,  from  its  beginning  or  end  in  the  brain  and  its  end  or 
beginning  in  its  appropriate  spinal  segment,  or  in  the  case  of  the 
median  posterior  tract  from  its  beginning  in  the  ganglion  of  a 
posterior  root  and  its  end  in  the  bulb  or  in  some  distant  spinal 
segment.  In  the  crossed  pyramidal  tract,  for  instance,  we  have 
reason  to  think  that  one  or  more  fibres  run  a  quite  unbroken  and 
isolated  course  from  the  cortex  of  the  cerebrum  through  various 
parts  of  the  brain,  along  the  whole  length  of  the  cord  until  they 
reach  the  lowermost  spinal  segmental  mechanism.  These  tracts 
serve  in  no  way  to  connect  one  segmental  mechanism  with  another. 
The  segmental  mechanisms  are  however  connected  together ;  and 
the  connections  between  them  seem  to  be  of  two  kinds.  In  the 
first  place,  as  we  have  already  suggested,  the  segmental  pieces  of 
grey  matter  are  so  fused  together  as  to  form  what  appears  to  be 
a  continuity  of  grey  matter  from  one  end  of  the  cord  to  the  other. 
Though  we  cannot  actually  track  our  way  histologically  through, 
and  are  still  less  aware  of  the  physiological  nature  of  the  labyrinth 
of  nerve-cells,  fibres  and  fibrils  which  make  up  what  we  have 
called  the  groundwork,  we  may  with  considerable  probability 
assume  that  the  passage  of  nervous  impulses  along  it  is  de- 
termined as  much  by  the  condition  of  the  material  as  by  its 
anatomical  disposition ;  that,  for  instance,  the  restrictions  to  the 
flow  of  an  impulse  are  brought  about  much  more  frequently 
by  the  refusal  of  the  molecules  of  nervous  matter  to  take  up  the 
molecular  disturbance  which  is  the  essence  of  the  impulse,  that 
is  to  say,  by  molecular  resistance,  than  by  actual  breaks  of  con- 
tinuity in  the  nervous  matter.  Indeed  we  have  some  reasons 
for  thinking  that  actual  structural  continuity  of  nervous  material 


CHAP.  L]  THE   SPINAL   CORD.  901 

is  not  essential  to  functional  continuity,  that  a  nerve  fibril  for 
instance  may  produce  its  due  effect  on  another  nerve  fibril  or 
on  a  nerve-cell,  if  sufficiently  in  contact  with  it,  though  the 
microscope  fails  to  demonstrate  actual  continuity. 

But  besides  the  grey  matter  there  are  areas  of  white  matter 
which  do  not  belong  either  to  the  nerve  roots  as  these  are  making 
their  way  into  the  grey  matter,  or  to  any  of  the  tracts  which  we 
have  mentioned.  These  comprise  the  strands  of  fibres  which  do 
not  undergo  either  ascending  or  descending  degeneration  when 
parts  of  the  spinal  cord  are  injured  or  diseased.  The  area  of 
white  matter  left  when  all  the  various  tracts  of  ascending  and 
descending  degeneration  detailed  above  are  taken  out,  seems,  at 
all  events  in  the  higher  parts  of  the  cord  (Fig.  104),  relatively 
small,  and  future  observations  may  continue  still  further  to  reduce 
it ;  but  it  must  be  remembered,  that  none  of  the  above-mentioned 
tracts  are  'pure';  they  are  all  more  or  less  mixed  up,  and  some 
largely  mixed  up,  with  fibres  which  do  not  degenerate.  Our  know- 
ledge is  at  present  too  scanty  to  allow  us  to  make  any  statement 
with  confidence  concerning  the  function  either  of  the  fibres 
forming  the  white  matter  not  yet  marked  out  into  tracts,  or  of  the 
fibres  scattered  among  the  acknowledged  tracts.  Bat  we  may,  at 
all  events  provisionally,  assume  that  these  fibres  serve  in  the  main 
as  commissures  connecting  the  successive  segmental  mechanisms 
with  each  other;  we  may  conclude  that  changes  taking  place  in 
one  segmental  mechanism  can  by  means  of  these  fibres  produce 
correlated  changes  in  some  other  distant  segmental  mechanism, 
without  calling  into  action  any  of  the  grey  matter  of  the  inter- 
vening segmental  mechanisms. 

The  commissures  which  we  may  suppose  to  be  thus  furnished 
by  white  matter  are  longitudinal  commissures  connecting  the 
segmental  mechanisms  of  the  same  lateral  half  of  the  spinal 
cord  with  each  other.  A  transverse  connection  between  the  two 
lateral  halves  is  afforded  in  some  measure  by  the  anterior  white 
commissure.  We  shall  see,  however,  later  on  reasons  for  thinking 
that  many  impulses  besides  those  passing  along  the  anterior 
commissure  cross  from  one  side  of  the  cord  to  the  other ;  and 
these  whether  they  pass  along  distinct  fibres  or  along  the  general 
groundwork  must  travel  by  the  grey  matter  of  the  isthmus  form- 
ing the  anterior  and  posterior  grey  commissures. 

Thus,  as  far  as  we  can  see  at  present,  the  spinal  cord  consists  of 
a  series  of  segmental  mechanisms  with  their  respective  afferent 
and  efferent  roots  (the  grey  matter  of  the  several  segments  being 
continuous  along  the  cord),  of  encephalic  ties  of  white  matter 
between  the  several  segments  and  the  brain,  of  longitudinal 
commissural  tracts  connecting  together  the  several  segmental 
mechanisms,  and  of  transverse  commissures  running  largely  in 
the  grey  matter. 


SEC.  3.     THE  REFLEX  ACTIONS  OF  THE 
SPINAL   COED. 


§  582.  In  the  preceding  portions  of  this  work  we  have  re- 
peatedly seen  that  though  we  can  learn  much  concerning  the 
working  of  an  organ,  or  tissue  or  part  of  the  body  by  studying  its 
behaviour  when  isolated  from  the  rest  of  the  body,  all  the  con- 
clusions thus  gained  have  to  be  checked  by  a  study  of  the 
behaviour  of  the  same  organ  or  part,  while  it  is  still  an  integral 
part  of  the  intact  body.  All  the  several  organs  and  tissues  are  so 
bound  together  by  various  ties,  that  the  actions  of  each  depend  on 
the  actions  of  the  rest ;  and  to  say  that  the  life  of  each  part  is  a 
function  of  the  life  of  the  whole,  is  no  less  true  than  to  say  that 
the  life  of  the  whole  is  a  function  of  the  life  of  each  part.  This  is 
especially  borne  in  upon  us,  when  we  come  to  study  the  actions  of 
the  central  nervous  system.  We  may,  on  anatomical  grounds, 
separate  the  spinal  cord  from  the  brain;  but  when  we  come  to 
consider  the  respective  functions  of  the  two,  we  are  brought  face 
to  face  with  the  fact  that  in  actual  life  a  large  part  of  the  work  of 
the  brain  is  carried  out  by  means  of  the  spinal  cord,  and  con- 
versely the  spinal  cord  does  its  work  habitually  under  the  influence 
of,  if  not  at  the  direct  bidding  of  the  brain.  We  may  gain  certain 
conclusions  by  studying  the  behaviour  of  the  spinal  cord  isolated 
from  the  brain,  or  of  parts  of  the  spinal  cord  isolated  from  each 
other;  but  we  must  be  even  more  cautious  than  when  we  were 
dealing  with  other  parts  of  the  body,  and  must  greatly  hesitate  to 
take  it  for  granted  that  the  work  which  we  can  make  the  spinal 
cord  or  a  part  of  the  spinal  cord  do,  when  isolated  from  the  brain, 
is  the  work  which  is  actually  done  in  the  intact  body  when  the 
brain  and  spinal  cord  form  an  unbroken  whole.  Moreover  this 
caution  becomes  increasingly  necessary,  when  in  our  studies  we 
pass  from  the  simpler  nervous  system  of  one  animal  to  the  more 
complex  nervous  system  of  another ;  for  it  is  by  the  complexity  of 
their  central  nervous  systems  more  than  by  any  thing  else,  that 
the  'highest'  animals  are  differentiated  from  those  'below'  them. 


CHAP,  i.]  THE   SPINAL   CORD.  903 

When  we  compare  a  rabbit,  a  dog,  a  monkey  and  a  man,  the 
differences  in  the  vascular,  digestive  and  respiratory  systems  of  the 
four,  striking  as  they  may  appear,  sink  into  insignificance  com- 
pared with  the  differences  exhibited  by  their  respective  central 
nervous  systems.  We  need  caution  when  from  the  results  of 
experiments  on  dogs  or  rabbits,  we  draw  conclusions  as  to  the 
digestion  or  circulation  of  man,  but  we  need  far  greater  caution 
when  from  the  behaviour  of  the  isolated  spinal  cord  of  one  of 
these  animals  we  infer  the  behaviour  of  the  intact  spinal  cord  of 
man. 

A  further  difficulty  meets  us  when  an  experimental  investiga- 
tion entails  operative  interference  with  the  central  nervous  system. 
Removal  or  section  of,  or  other  injury  to  parts  of  the  brain  or 
spinal  cord  is  very  apt  to  give  rise  in  varying  degree  to  what  is 
known  as  '  shock.'  The  cutting  or  tearing  or  other  lesion  of  any 
considerable  mass  of  nervous  substance  affects  the  activity,  not 
only  of  the  structures  immediately  injured,  but  of  other,  it  may 
be  far  distant,  structures.  The  nature  of  'shock'  is  not  as  yet 
thoroughly  understood,  but  may  perhaps,  in  part  at  all  events,  be 
explained  by  regarding  the  lesion  as  a  very  powerful  stimulus, 
which,  partly  by  way  of  inhibition  but  still  more  by  way  of 
exhaustion,  depresses  or  suspends  for  a  while  normal  functions, 
and  thus  gives  rise  to  temporary  diminution  or  loss  of  conscious- 
ness, of  volition,  of  reflex  movements  and  other  nervous  actions. 
Thus  a  section  through  the  spinal  cord,  even  when  made  with 
the  sharpest  instrument  and  with  the  utmost  skill,  so  as  to  avoid 
all  bruising  as  much  as  possible,  may  for  a  while  suspend  all 
reflex  activity  of  the  cord,  or  indeed  all  the  obvious  activities  of 
the  whole  central  nervous  system.  We  may  add  that  such  a 
'  shock '  of  the  central  nervous  system  may  also  be  produced  by 
sudden  lesions  not  bearing  directly  on  the  central  nervous  system, 
as  for  instance  by  extensive  injury  to  a  limb. 

Moreover  in  many  cases  in  which  the  effects  of  experimental 
interference  have  been  watched  for  some  considerable  time,  days, 
months  or  years  after  the  operation,  it  has  been  observed,  on  the 
one  hand,  that  phenomena  which  are  conspicuous  in  the  early 
period  may  eventually  disappear,  and,  on  the  other  hand,  that 
activities  which  are  at  first  absent  may  later  on  make  their 
appearance ;  movements  for  instance  which  are  at  first  frequent 
after  a  while  die  away,  and  conversely,  movements  which  at  first 
seemed  impossible  are  later  on  easily  achieved.  We  have  to 
distinguish  or  to  attempt  to  distinguish  between  the  temporary 
and  the  lasting  effects  of  the  operation,  including  among  the 
former  not  only  those  of  ordinary  'shock,'  bat  others  of  slower 
development  or  longer  duration.  In  many  instances  where  a  part 
of  the  central  nervous  system  is  by  section  or  otherwise  suddenly 
separated  from  the  rest,  the  phenomena  suggest  that  the  separated 
part  is  at  first  profoundly  influenced  as  to  its  activities  by  the 


904  REFLEX   ACTIONS.  [BOOK  in. 

withdrawal  of  various  influences  which  previously  were  being- 
exerted  upon  it  by  the  rest  of  the  system,  but  later  on  accom- 
modates itself  to  its  new  conditions,  and  learns,  so  to  speak,  to 
act  without  the  help  of  those  influences.  And  indeed  it  is  possible 
that  some  of  the  effects  of  even  immediate  '  shock '  may  be  due, 
not,  as  suggested  above,  to  the  action  of  an  inhibitory  or  exhausting 
stimulus,  but  to  the  sudden  cessation  of  habitual  influences. 

Still,  in  spite  of  all  these  difficulties,  it  is  possible  not  only  to 
ascertain  the  working  of  an  isolated  portion  of  the  central  nervous 
system,  but  even  to  infer  from  the  results  some  conclusions  as  to 
the  share  taken  by  that  portion  in  the  working  of  the  entire  and 
intact  system.  There  can  be  no  doubt,  for  instance,  that  the 
spinal  cord  can,  quite  apart  from  the  brain,  carry  out  various  reflex 
actions,  and  that  moreover  it  does  carry  out  actions  of  this  kind 
when  in  the  intact  organism  it  is  working  in  concert  with  the 
brain.  Indeed  the  carrying  out  of  various  reflex  actions  seems  to 
be  one  of  the  most  important  functions  of  the  spinal  cord,  so 
much  so  that,  though  the  brain  or,  at  least,  parts  of  the  brain  can 
also  and  do  develope  reflex  actions,  the  spinal  cord  offers  the  best 
field  for  the  study  of  these  actions.  We  have  already  (§  101) 
touched  on  the  general  features  of  reflex  actions,  and  elsewhere 
have  incidentally  dwelt  on  particular  instances ;  we  may  therefore 
confine  ourselves  now  to  certain  points  of  special  interest. 

§  583.  Reflex  movements  are  perhaps  best  studied  in  the 
frog  and  other  cold-blooded  animals,  since  in  these  the  actions 
of  the  cord  are  less  dependent  on,  and  hence  less  obscured  by 
the  working  of,  the  other  parts  of  the  central  nervous  system. 
They  obtain  however  in  the  warm-blooded  mammal  also,  but  in 
these  special  precautions  are  necessary  to  secure  their  full 
development.  In  the  frog  the  shock,  which  as  we  have  said 
follows  upon  division  of  the  spinal  cord  and  for  a  while  suspends 
reflex  activity,  soon  passes  away;  within  a  very  short  time  after 
the  bulb  for  instance  has  been  divided  the  most  complicated  reflex 
movements  can  be  carried  on  by  the  frog's  spinal  cord  when  the 
appropriate  stimuli  are  applied.  With  the  mammal  the  case  is 
very  different.  For  days  even  after  division  of  the  spinal  cord  the 
parts  of  the  body  supplied  by  nerves  springing  from  the  cord  below 
the  section  may  exhibit  very  feeble  reactions  only.  In  the  dog, 
for  instance,  after  division  of  the  spinal  cord  in  the  lower  dorsal 
region,  the  hind  limbs  hang  flaccid  and  motionless,  and  pinching 
the  hind  foot  evokes  as  a  response  either  slight  irregular  movements 
or  none  at  all.  Indeed  were  our  observations  limited  to  this  period 
we  might  infer  that  the  reflex  actions  of  the  spinal  cord  in  the 
mammal  were  but  feeble  and  insignificant.  If  however  the  animal 
be  kept  alive  for  a  longer  period,  for  weeks  or  better  still  for 
months,  though  no  union  or  regeneration  of  the  spinal  cord  takes 
place,  reflex  movements  of  a  powerful,  varied  and  complex  character 
manifest  themselves  in  the  hind  limbs  and  hinder  parts  of  the 


CHAP,  i.]  THE   SPINAL   CORD.  905 

body ;  a  very  feeble  stimulus  applied  to  the  skin  of  these  regions 
promptly  gives  rise  to  extensive  and  yet  coordinate  movements. 
Indeed  the  more  the  matter  is  studied,  the  stronger  is  the 
evidence  that  the  reflex  movements  carried  out  by  isolated 
portions  of  the  spinal  cord  of  the  mammal  are  hardly  less  definite, 
complete  and  purposeful,  than  those  witnessed  in  the  frog.  It  is 
worthy  of  attention,  as  bearing  out  the  remarks  made  above  on 
the  great  differentiation  of  the  central  nervous  system  in  the 
higher  animals,  that  the  reflex  phenomena  in  mammals  vary  very 
much  not  only  in  different  species  but  also  in  different  individuals 
and  in  the  same  individual  under  different  circumstances.  Race, 
age,  and  previous  training,  seem  to  have  a  marked  effect  in 
determining  the  extent  and  character  of  the  reflex  actions  which 
the  spinal  cord  is  capable  of  carrying  out ;  and  these  seem  also 
to  be  largely  influenced  by  passing  circumstances,  such  as  whether 
food  has  been  recently  taken  or  no.  It  has  been  asserted  that  the 
isolated  spinal  cord  of  the  rabbit,  which  has  been  the  subject  of  so 
many  experiments,  is,  as  compared  with  that  of  the  dog  and 
many  other  mammals,  singularly  deficient  in  the  power  of  carrying 
out  complex  reflex  movements. 

In  studying  reflex  actions  in  man  we  are  met  with  the 
difficulty  that  we  never  have  to  deal  with  a  portion  of  the  spinal 
cord  separated  from  the  rest  of  the  central  nervous  system  under 
the  favourable  circumstances  of  experimental  investigation.  In 
man,  we  must  be  content  to  examine  reflex  actions  either  while 
the  whole  nervous  system  is  intact,  or  when  a  portion  of  the  cord 
has  been  wholly  or  partially  separated  by  some  more  or  less  diffuse 
disease  or  by  some  accident  involving  more  or  less  crushing  of  the 
nervous  structures.  Hence,  the  caution  already  given,  as  to 
drawing  inferences  concerning  man  from  the  results  of  experi- 
ments on  animals,  acquires  still  greater  force. 

§  584.  Confining  ourselves  at  first  to  the  results  of  experi- 
ments on  animals  we  may  say  that  in  both  cold-blooded  and 
warm-blooded  animals  the  salient  feature  of  ordinary  reflex 
actions  is  their  purposeful  character,  though  every  variety  of 
movement  may  be  witnessed,  from  a  simple  spasm  to  a  most 
complex  manoeuvre.  And  in  all  reflex  movements,  both  simple 
and  complex,  we  can  recognize  certain  determining  influences 
which  more  or  less  directly  contribute  to  the  shaping  of  this 
purposeful  character. 

Thus  the  features  of  any  movement  taking  place  as  part  of 
a  reflex  action  are  in  part  determined  by  the  characters  of  the 
afferent  impulses.  Simple  nervous  impulses  generated  by  the 
direct  stimulation  of  afferent  nerve  fibres  generally  evoke  as  reflex 
movements  merely  irregular  spasms  in  a  few  muscles ;  whereas 
the  more  complicated  differentiated  sensory  impulses  generated 
by  the  application  of  the  stimulus  to  the  skin,  readily  give  rise 
to  large  and  purposeful  movements.  It  is  easier  to  produce  a 


906  REFLEX  ACTIONS.  [BOOK  in. 

complex  reflex  action  by  a  slight  pressure  on  or  other  stimulation 
of  the  skin  than  by  even  strong  induction-shocks  applied  directly 
to  a  nerve  trunk.  If,  in  a  brainless  frog,  the  area  of  skin  supplied 
by  one  of  the  dorsal  cutaneous  nerves  be  separated  by  section 
from  the  rest  of  the  skin  of  the  back,  the  nerve  being  left  attached 
to  the  piece  of  skin  and  carefully  protected  from  injury,  it  will  be 
found  that  slight  stimuli  applied  to  the  surface  of  the  piece  of 
skin  easily  evoke  reflex  actions,  whereas  the  trunk  of  the  nerve 
may  be  stimulated  with  even  strong  currents  without  producing 
anything  more  than  irregular  movements.  In  ordinary  mechanical 
and  chemical  stimulation  of  the  skin  it  is  not  a  single  impulse  but 
a  series  of  impulses  which  passes  upwards  along  the  sensory  nerve, 
the  changes  in  which  may  be  compared  to  the  changes  in  a  motor 
nerve  during  tetanus.  In  every  reflex  action,  in  fact,  the  central 
mechanism  may  be  looked  upon  as  being  thrown  into  activity 
through  a  summation  of  the  afferent  impulses  reaching  it.  Hence 
while  a  reflex  action  is  readily  called  forth  by  even  feeble  induction- 
shocks  applied  to  the  skin  if  they  be  repeated  sufficiently  rapidly, 
a  solitary  induction-shock  is  ineffectual  unless  it  be  strong  enough 
to  cause  in  the  skin  or  nerves  changes  of  an  electrolytic  nature 
sufficient  to  give  rise  of  themselves  to  a  series  of  impulses. 

§  585.  When  a  muscle  is  thrown  into  contraction  in  a  reflex 
action,  the  pitch  of  the  sound  which  it  gives  forth  does  not  vary 
with  the  stimulus,  but  is  constant,  being  the  same  as  that  given 
forth  by  a  muscle  thrown  into  contraction  by  the  will.  From 
which  we  infer,  even  bearing  in  mind  the  discussion  in  §  80 
concerning  the  nature  of  the  muscular  sound,  that  in  a  reflex 
action  the  afferent  impulses  do  not  simply  pass  through  the  centre 
in  the  same  way  that  they  pass  along  afferent  nerves,  but  are 
profoundly  modified.  And  in  accordance  with  this  we  find,  as  we 
shall  see,  that  a  reflex  action  takes  up  an  amount  of  time,  the 
greater  part  of  which  is  spent  in  the  carrying  out  of  the  central 
changes,  and  which  though  variable  is  always  much  longer,  and 
may  be  very  much  longer,  than  that  taken  up  by  the  mere  passage 
of  a  nervous  impulse  along  a  corresponding  length  of  nerve  fibre. 
The  term  reflex  action  is  therefore  an  unsuitable  one.  The 
afferent  impulse  is  not  simply  reflected  or  turned  aside  into  an 
efferent  channel ;  on  its  arrival  at  the  centre  it  starts  changes  of 
a  different  nature  from  and  more  complex  than  its  own ;  and  the 
issue  of  efferent  impulse  is  the  result  of  those  more  complex 
changes,  not  the  mere  continuation  of  the  simpler  afferent  impulse. 
In  other  words,  the  interval  between  the  advent  at  the  central 
organ  of  afferent,  and  the  exit  from  it  of  efferent  impulses,  is  a 
busy  time  for  the  nervous  substance  of  that  organ;  dnring  it 
many  processes,  of  which  we  have  at  present  very  little  exact 
knowledge,  are  being  carried  on. 

§  586.  The  character  of  the  movement  forming  part  of  a 
reflex  action  is  also  influenced  by  the  intensity  of  the  stimulus.  A 


CHAP,  i.]  THE   SPINAL   CORD.  907 

slight  stimulus,  such  as  gentle  contact  of  the  skin  with  some  body, 
will  produce  one  kind  of  movement ;  and  a  strong  stimulus,  such 
as  a  sharp  prick  applied  to  the  same  spot  of  skin,  will  call  forth 
quite  a  different  movement.  When  a  decapitated  snake  or  newt 
is  suspended  and  the  skin  of  the  tail  lightly  touched  with  the 
finger,  the  tail  bends  towards  the  finger ;  when  the  skin  is  pricked 
or  burnt,  the  tail  is  turned  away  from  the  offending  object.  And 
so  in  many  other  instances.  It  must  be  remembered  of  course 
that  a  difference  in  the  intensity  of  the  stimulus  entails  a 
difference  in  the  characters  of  the  afferent  impulses;  gentle 
contact  gives  rise  to  what  we  call  a  sensation  of  touch,  while  a 
sharp  prick  gives  rise  to  pain,  consciousness  being  differently 
affected  in  the  two  cases  because  the  afferent  impulses  are 
different.  Hence  the  instances  in  question  are  in  reality  fuller 
illustrations  of  the  dependence,  to  which  we  called  attention  above, 
of  the  characters  of  a  reflex  movement  on  the  characters  of  the 
afferent  impulses. 

Further,  as  we  have  already  pointed  out  (§  101)  while  the 
motor  impulses  started  by  a  weak  stimulus  applied  to  an 
afferent  nerve  are  transmitted  along  a  few,  those  started  by  a 
strong  stimulus  may  spread  to  many  efferent  nerves.  Granting 
that  any  particular  afferent  nerve  is  more  especially  associated  with 
certain  efferent  nerves  than  with  any  others,  so  that  the  reflex 
impulses  generated  by  afferent  impulses  entering  the  cord  by  the 
former  pass  with  the  least  resistance  down  the  latter,  we  must 
evidently  admit  further  that  other  efferent  nerves  are  also,  though 
less  directly,  connected  with  the  same  afferent  nerve,  the  passage 
into  the  second  efferent  nerve  meeting  with  a  greater  but  not  an 
insuperable  resistance.  When  a  frog  is  poisoned  with  strychnia, 
a  slight  touch  on  any  part  of  the  skin  may  cause  convulsions  of  the 
whole  body;  that  is  to  say,  the  afferent  impulses  passing  along  any 
single  afferent  nerve  may  give  rise  to  the  discharge  of  efferent  im- 
pulses along  any  or  all  of  the  efferent  nerves.  This  proves  that  a 
physiological  if  not  an  anatomical  continuity  obtains  between  all 
the  parts  of  the  spinal  cord  which  are  concerned  in  reflex  action, 
that  the  nervous  network  intervening  between  the  afferent  and 
efferent  fibres  forms  along  the  whole  length  of  the  cord  a 
functionally  continuous  field.  This  continuous  network  however 
we  must  suppose  to  be  marked  out  into  tracts  presenting 
greater  or  less  resistance  to  the  progress  of  the  impulses  into 
which  afferent  impulses,  coming  along  this  or  that  afferent  nerve, 
are  transformed  on  their  advent  at  the  network ;  and  accordingly 
the  path  of  any  series  of  impulses  in  the  network  will  be  deter- 
mined largely  by  the  energy  of  the  afferent  impulses.  And  the 
action  of  strychnia  may  be  in  part  explained  by  supposing  that 
it  reduces  and  equalises  the  normal  resistance  of  this  network,  so 
that  even  weak  impulses  travel  over  all  its  tracts  with  great  ease. 

§  587.     Further,   the   movement,   forming   part   of    a   reflex 


908  REFLEX   ACTIONS.  [BOOK  in. 

action,  varies  in  character  according  to  the  particular  part  of  the 
body  to  which  the  stimulus  is  applied.  The  reflex  actions 
developed  by  stimulation  of  the  internal  viscera  are  different 
from  those  excited  by  stimulation  of  the  skin.  We  have  reason 
to  think  that  the  contraction  of  or  other  changes  in  a  skeletal 
muscle  may  produce,  by  reflex  action,  contractions  of  other  muscles ; 
and  such  reflex  actions  also  differ  from  those  started  by  stimulation 
of  the  skin.  In  reflex  actions  started  by  applying  a  stimulus  to 
the  skin  the  movements  vary  largely  according  to  the  particular 
area  of  the  skin  which  is  affected.  Thus,  pinching  the  folds  of 
skin  surrounding  the  anus  of  the  frog  produces  different  effects 
from  those  witnessed  when  the  flank  or  toe  is  pinched ;  and, 
speaking  generally,  the  stimulation  of  a  particular  spot  calls 
forth  particular  movements.  In  the  case  of  the  simpler  reflex 
movements,  it  appears  to  be  a  general  rule  that  a  movement 
started  by  the  stimulation  of  a  sensory  surface  or  region  on  one 
side  of  the  body,  is  developed  on  the  same  side  of  the  body,  and 
if  it  spreads  to  the  other  side,  still  remains  most  intense  on 
the  same  side ;  the  movement  on  the  other  side  moreover  is 
symmetrical  with  that  on  the  same  side.  It  has  been  main- 
tained that  'crossed'  or  diagonal  reflex  movements,  as  where 
stimulation  of  one  fore-foot  leads  to  movements  of  the  opposite 
hind-limb,  do  not  occur  unless  some  portion  of  the  bulb  be  left 
attached  to  the  spinal  cord.  Seeing  that  locomotion  in  four- 
footed  animals  is  largely  effected  by  diagonal  movements  of  the 
limbs,  one  would  rather  have  expected  to  find  the  spinal  cord 
itself  provided  with  mechanisms  to  assist  in  carrying  them  out ; 
and  indeed  it  is  affirmed  that  in  the  case  of  cold-blooded  animals 
and  of  many  young  mammals,  after  division  of  the  spinal  cord 
below  the  bulb,  a  gentle  stimulation  will  provoke  a  diagonal 
movement,  slight  pressure  on  one  fore-foot  for  example  giving 
rise  to  movements  in  the  opposite  hind-leg;  a  strong  stimulus 
however  will  produce  an  ordinary  one-sided  movement.  Again, 
when  in  a  dog  the  cord  has  been  divided  in  the  lower  thoracic 
region  so  that  the  hind  limbs  depend  on  the  lumbar  cord  alone,  a 
rhythmically  repeated  drawing  up  and  letting  down  of  the  hind 
limbs  is  witnessed  when  these  are  allowed  to  hang  down ;  and 
these  movements,  which  appear  to  be  of  a  reflex  nature  excited 
by  the  pendant  position  of  the  limbs,  are  often  seen  to  alternate 
regularly  in  the  two  limbs,  the  right  leg  being  extended  while  the 
left  leg  is  being  drawn  up  and  vice  versa.  It  may  further  be 
observed  that  if  the  foot  of  one  pendant  limb  be  pinched  while 
the  other  limb  is  passively  flexed  the  flexion  of  the  limb  which  is 
pinched  is  accompanied  by  an  extension  of  the  other  limb.  In 
these  respects  however  different  animals,  as  already  urged,  differ 
from  each  other. 

§  588.     From  these  and  similar  phenomena  we  may  infer  that 
the  nervous  network  spoken  of  above  is,  so  to  speak,  mapped  out 


CHAP,  i.]  THE   SPINAL   CORD.  909 

into  nervous  mechanisms  by  the  establishment  of  lines  of  greater 
or  less  resistance,  so  that  the  disturbances  in  it  generated  by 
certain  afferent  impulses  are  directed  into  certain  efferent  channels. 
It  may  be  added  that  though  conspicuously  purposeful  movements 
seem  to  need  the  concurrent  action  of  several  segments  of  the  cord, 
and  as  a  rule,  the  greater  the  length  of  the  cord  involved  the 
more  complex  and  the  more  distinctly  purposeful  the  movement, 
still  the  movements  evoked  by  even  a  segment  of  the  cord  may 
be  purposeful  in  character ;  hence  we  must  conclude  that  every 
segment  of  the  nervous  network  is  mapped  out  into  mechanisms. 
But  the  arrangement  of  these  mechanisms,  especially  of  the  more 
complex  ones,  is  not  a  fixed  and  rigid  one.  We  cannot  always 
predict  exactly  the  nature  of  the  movement  which  will  result  from 
the  stimulation  of  any  particular  spot,  because  the  result  will  vary 
according  to  the  condition  of  the  spinal  cord,  especially  in  relation 
to  the  strength  and  character  of  the  stimulus.  Moreover,  under 
a  change  of  circumstances  a  movement  quite  different  from  the 
normal  one  may  make  its  appearance.  Thus  when  a  drop  of  acid 
is  placed  on  the  right  flank  of  a  brainless  frog,  the  right  foot  is 
almost  invariably  used  to  rub  off  the  acid ;  in  this  there  appears 
nothing  more  than  a  mere  'mechanical'  reflex  action.  If  however 
the  right  leg  be  cut  off,  or  the  right  foot  be  otherwise  hindered 
from  rubbing  off  the  acid,  the  left  foot  is,  under  the  exceptional 
circumstances,  used  for  the  purpose.  This  at  first  sight  looks 
like  an  intelligent  choice.  A  choice  it  evidently  is ;  and  were 
there  many  instances  of  choice,  and  were  there  any  evidence 
of  a  variable  automatism,  like  that  which  we  call  'volition,' 
being  manifested  by  the  spinal  cord  of  the  frog,  we  should  be 
justified  in  supposing  that  the  choice  was  determined  by  an 
intelligence.  But,  as  we  shall  have  occasion  later  on  to  point  out, 
a  frog,  deprived  of  its  brain  so  that  the  spinal  cord  only  is  left, 
makes  no  spontaneous  movements  at  all.  Such  an  entire  absence 
of  spontaneity  is  wholly  inconsistent  with  the  possession  of 
intelligence.  Then  again  the  above  experiment,  if  not  the  only 
instance,  is  at  all  events  by  far  the  most  striking  instance  of  choice 
on  the  part  of  a  brainless  frog.  We  are  therefore  led  to  conclude 
that  the  phenomena  must  be  explained  in  some  other  way  than 
by  being  referred  to  the  working  of  an  intelligence.  Moreover 
this  conclusion  is  supported  by  the  behaviour  of  other  animals. 
Thus  similar  vicarious  reflex  movements  may  be  witnessed  in 
mammals,  though  not  perhaps  to  such  a  striking  extent  as  in  frogs. 
In  dogs,  in  which  partial  removal  of  the  cerebral  hemispheres  has 
apparently  heightened  the  reflex  excitability  of  the  spinal  cord, 
the  remarkable  scratching  movements  of  the  hind  leg  which  are 
called  forth  by  stimulating  a  particular  spot  on  the  loins  or  side  of 
the  body,  are  executed  by  the  leg  of  the  opposite  side,  if  the  leg  of 
the  same  side  be  gently  held.  In  this  case  the  vicarious  movements 
are  ineffectual,  the  leg  not  being,  as  in  the  case  of  the  frog,  crossed 


910  REFLEX   ACTIONS.  [BOOK  m. 

over  so  as  to  bear  on  the  spot  stimulated,  and  cannot  be  considered 
as  betokening  intelligence.  Again  the  'mechanical'  nature  of 
reflex  actions  is  well  illustrated  by  the  behaviour  of  a  decapitated 
snake.  When  the  body  of  the  animal  in  this  condition  is  brought 
into  contact  at  several  places  at  once  with  an  arm  or  a  stick, 
complex  reflex  movements  are  excited,  the  obvious  purpose  as  well 
as  effect  of  which  is  to  twine  the  body  round  the  object.  A 
decapitated  snake  will  however  with  equal  and  fatal  readiness 
twine  itself  round  a  red-hot  bar  of  iron,  which  is  made  to  touch  its 
skin  in  several  places  at  the  same  time. 

§  589.  In  considering  the  nature  of  the  events  in  the  spinal 
cord  which  determine  the  behaviour  of  the  frog  in  the  instance 
just  mentioned  we  must  bear  in  mind  that  the  movements  in 
question  are  'coordinated;'  that  is  to  say  not  only  are  many 
distinct  muscles  brought  into  play  but  certain  relations  are 
maintained  between  the  amount,  duration  and  exact  time  of 
occurrence  of  the  contraction  of  each  muscle  and  those  of  the 
contractions  of  its  fellow  muscles  sharing  in  the  movement.  In 
the  absence  of  such  coordination  the  movement  would  become 
irregular  and  ineffectual.  We  shall  have  occasion  later  on  in 
dealing  with  voluntary  movements  to  point  out  that  the  coordina- 
tion and  hence  the  due  accomplishment  of  a  voluntary  movement 
is  dependent  on  certain  afferent  impulses  passing  up  from  the 
contracting  muscles  to  the  central  nervous  system,  and  guiding  the 
discharge  of  the  efferent  impulses  which  call  forth  the  contractions. 
When  these  afferent  impulses  affect  consciousness  we  speak  of 
them  as  constituting  a  'muscular  sense;'  it  is,  as  we  shall  see,  by 
the  '  muscular  sense'  that  we  become  aware  of  and  can  appreciate 
the  condition  of  our  muscles.  But  we  have  reason  to  think  that 
the  afferent  impulses  which  constitute  the  basis  of  the  muscular 
sense,  whatever  be  their  exact  nature,  in  order  to  play  their  part 
in  bringing  about  the  coordination  of  a  voluntary  movement  need 
not  pass  right  up  to  the  brain  and  develope  a  distinct  muscular 
'  sense,'  but  may  produce  their  effect  by  working  on  the  nervous 
mechanisms  of  the  spinal  cord  with  which .  the  motor  fibres 
carrying  out  the  movement  are  connected.  In  other  words,  the 
coordination  of  a  voluntary  movement  takes  place  in  the  part  of 
the  spinal  cord  which  carries  out  the  movement,  and  not  in  the 
brain,  though  the  latter  may  be  conscious  of  the  whole  movement 
including  its  coordination. 

But  if  the  spinal  cord  possesses  mechanisms  for  carrying  out 
coordinated  movements,  which  in  the  case  of  voluntary  move- 
ments are  discharged  by  nervous  impulses  descending  from  the 
brain,  we  may  infer  that  in  reflex  actions  the  same  mechanisms 
are  brought  into  action  though  they  are  discharged  by  afferent 
impulses  coming  along  afferent  nerves  instead  of  by  impulses 
descending  from  the  brain.  The  movements  of  reflex  origin, 
in  all  their  features  except  their  exciting  cause,  appear  identical 


CHAP,  i.]  THE   SPINAL   CORD.  911 

with  voluntary  movements;  the  two  can  only  be  distinguished 
from  each  other  by  a  knowledge  of  the  exciting  cause.  And  it 
seems  unreasonable  to  suppose  that  the  spinal  cord  should  possess 
two  sets  of  mechanisms  in  all  respects  identical  save  that  the  one 
is  discharged  by  volitional  impulses  from  the  brain  and  the  other 
by  afferent  impulses  from  afferent  nerves. 

We  are  led  therefore  to  the  conclusion  that  in  a  reflex  action 
two  kinds  of  afferent  impulses  are  concerned  :  the  ordinary  afferent 
impulses  which  discharge  the  nervous  mechanism  within  the  cord 
and  so  provoke  the  movement,  and  the  afferent  impulses  which 
connect  that  nervous  mechanism  with  the  muscles  about  to  be 
called  into  play,  and  which  take  part  in  the  coordination  of  the 
movement  provoked.  The  nature  of  these  latter  afferent  impulses 
is  at  present  obscure ;  we  know  as  yet  little  more  than  the  fact  of 
their  existence ;  but  if  we  admit,  as  we  seem  compelled  to  do, 
that  the  character  of  a  reflex  action  is  determined  by  them  as  well 
as  by  the  afferent  impulses  which  actually  discharge  the  mecha- 
nism, it  seems  possible  that  a  fuller  knowledge  of  these  coordinating 
afferent  impulses  may  afford  an  adequate  explanation  of  the  fact 
that  when,  as  in  the  case  of  the  frog  in  question,  the  usual  set  of 
muscles  cannot  be  employed  by  the  nervous  mechanism,  recourse 
is  had  to  another  set. 

We  have  avoided  the  introduction  of  the  word  '  consciousness ' 
as  unnecessarily  complicating  the  question :  and  it  would  be  out 
of  place  to  discuss  psychological  problems  here.  We  may  remark 
however  that  since  we  have  no  objective  proofs  of  consciousness 
outside  ourselves,  and  only  infer  by  analogy  that  such  and  such  an 
act  is  an  outcome  of  consciousness  on  account  of  its  likeness  to 
acts  which  are  the  outcome  of  our  own  consciousness,  we  conclude 
that  the  brainless  frog  possesses  no  active  consciousness  like  our 
own,  because  absence  of  spontaneous  movements  seems  to  be 
irreconcilable  with  the  existence  of  an  active  consciousness  whose 
very  essence  is  a  series  of  changes.  Consciousness  as  we  recognize 
it  seems  to  be  necessarily  operating  as,  or  to  be  indissolubly 
associated  with  the  presence  of,  an  incessantly  repeated  internal 
stimulus ;  and  we  cannot  conceive  of  that  stimulus  failing  to 
excite  mechanisms  of  movement  which,  as  in  the  case  of  the 
brainless  frog,  are  confessedly  present.  We  may  however  distin- 
guish between  an  active  continuous  consciousness,  such  as  we 
usually  understand  by  the  term,  and  a  passing  or  momentary 
condition,  which  we  may  speak  of  as  consciousness,  but  which  is 
wholly  discontinuous  from  an  antecedent  or  from  a  subsequent 
similar  momentary  condition;  and  indeed  we  may  suppose  that 
the  complete  consciousness  of  ourselves,  and  the  similarly  com- 
plete consciousness  which  we  infer  to  exist  in  many  animals,  has 
been  gradually  evolved  out  of  such  a  rudimentary  consciousness. 
We  may,  on  this  view,  suppose  that  every  nervous  action  of  a 
certain  intensity  or  character  is  accompanied  by  some  amount 

F.  58 


912  REFLEX   ACTIONS.  [BOOK  m. 

of  consciousness,  which  we  may,  in  a  way,  compare  to  the 
light  emitted  when  a  combustion  previously  giving  rise  to 
invisible  heat  waxes  fiercer.  We  may  thus  infer  that  when  the 
brainless  frog  is  stirred  by  some  stimulus  to  a  reflex  act,  the 
spinal  cord  is  lit  up  by  a  momentary  flash  of  consciousness 
coming  out  of  darkness  and  dying  away  into  darkness  again  ;  and 
we  may  perhaps  further  infer  that  such  a  passing  consciousness  is 
the  better  developed  ^the  larger  the  portion  of  the  cord  involved  in 
the  reflex  act  and  the  more  complex  the  movement.  But  such  a 
momentary  flash,  even  if  we  admit  its  existence,  is  something 
very  different  from  consciousness  as  ordinarily  understood,  is  far 
removed  from  intelligence,  and  cannot  be  appealed  to  as  explaining 
the  '  choice '  spoken  of  above. 

§  590.  Lastly,  the  characters  of  a  reflex  movement  are,  as  we 
need  hardly  say,  dependent  on  the  intrinsic  condition  of  the  cord. 
The  action  of  strychnia  just  alluded  to  is  an  instance  of  an 
apparent  augmentation  of  reflex  action  best  explained  by  supposing 
that  the  resistances  in  the  cord  are  lessened.  There  are  probably 
however  cases  in  which  the  explosive  energy  of  the  nervous 
substance  is  positively  increased  above  the  normal.  Conversely, 
by  various  influences  of  a  depressing  character,  as  by  various 
anaesthetics  or  other  poisons,  reflex  action  may  be  lessened  or 
prevented ;  and  this  again  may  arise  either  from  an  increase  of 
resistance,  or  from  a  diminution  in  the  actual  discharge  of  energy. 
So  also,  various  diseases  may  so  affect  the  spinal  cord  as  to  produce 
on  the  one  hand  increased  reflex  excitability  so  that  a  mere  touch 
may  produce  a  violent  movement,  and  on  the  other  hand  diminished 
reflex  excitability  so  that  it  becomes  difficult  or  impossible  to  call 
forth  a  reflex  action. 

§  591.  When  we  come  to  study  the  reflex  actions  of  man  we 
should  at  first  perhaps  be  inclined  to  infer  that,  since  in  him  the 
spinal  cord  is  so  largely  used  as  the  instrument  of  the  brain,  the 
independent  reflex  actions  of  the  cord,  at  least  such  as  affect 
skeletal  muscles,  are  in  him  of  much  less  importance  than  they 
appear  to  be  in  animals;  and  experience  seems  to  support  this 
view.  But  it  must  be  remembered  that  in  his  case,  as  we  have 
already  stated  (§  583),  we  lack  the  guidance  of  experimental  results; 
we  are  obliged  to  trust  to  the  entangled  phenomena  of  disease  or 
to  a  study  of  the  behaviour  of  the  cord  while  it  is  still  a  part  of 
an  intact  nervous  system ;  and  each  of  these  methods  presents 
difficulties  of  its  own.  The  movements,  which  in  the  intact  human 
body  we  can  recognize  as  indubitable  reflex  actions,  are  as  a  rule 
simple  and  unimportant.  They  are,  in  by  far  the  greater  number 
of  instances,  occasioned  by  stimulation  of  the  skin  or  of  the  mucous 
membrane,  for  the  most  part  involve  a  few  muscles  only,  and  rarely 
indicate  any  very  complex  coordination.  The  flexion,  followed  by 
extension,  of  the  leg  which  is  called  forth  by  tickling  the  sole  of 
the  foot,  or  the  winking  of  the  eye  when  the  cornea  or  conjunctiva 


CHAP,  i.]  THE   SPINAL   CORD.  913 

is  touched,  may  perhaps  be  regarded  as  the  type  of  these  move- 
ments. A  very  common  form  of  reflex  action  is  that  in  which  a 
muscle  or  group  of  muscles  is  thrown  into  contraction  by  stimula- 
tion of  the  overlying  or  neighbouring  skin,  as  when  the  abdominal 
muscles  contract  upon  stroking  the  skin  of  the  abdomen  or  the 
testicle  is  retracted  upon  stroking  the  inside  of  the  thigh.  A 
reflex  movement  may  occur  as  the  result  of  stimulation  of  an  organ 
of  special  sense,  parts  of  the  central  nervous  system  other  than 
the  spinal  cord  serving  as  the  centre.  A  sound  or  a  flash  of 
light  readily  produces  a  start,  a  bright  light  makes  the  eye  wink 
and  may  cause  a  person  to  sneeze  (the  greater  coordination 
manifest  in  this  act  being  due  to  the  fact  that  the  complex  res- 
piratory mechanism  is  brought  into  play,  §  391),  and  reflex  move- 
ments may  result  from  a  taste  or  smell.  A  special  form  of 
reflex  action,  or  at  least  an  action  resembling  a  reflex  action,  is 
called  forth  by  sharply  striking  certain  tendons;  for  instance 
striking  the  tendon  below  the  patella  gives  rise  to  a  sudden 
extension  of  the  leg,  known  as  the  "knee-jerk";  but  it  will  be 
best  to  discuss  these  '  tendon  reflexes '  or  '  muscle  reflexes  '  as  they 
are  called  later  on  in  another  connection. 

On  the  whole  the  reflex  movements  carried  out  by  the  intact 
nervous  system  of  man  are  we  repeat  scanty  and  comparatively 
simple ;  but  we  are  not  j  ustified  in  inferring  from  this  that  the 
human  spinal  cord,  left  to  itself,  is  incapable  of  doing  more,  that 
owing  to  the  predominant  activity  of  the  brain  it  has  lost  the 
powers  possessed  by  the  spinal  cord  in  the  lower  animals.  For  it 
may  be  that  the  cord,  when  joined  to  the  brain,  is  through  various 
influences  proceeding  from  the  latter  in  a  different  condition  from 
that  in  which  it  is  when  separated  from  the  brain;  indeed  we 
have  reason  to  think  that  this  is  so ;  and  we  may  here  remark 
that  in  the  lower  animals,  as  in  man,  the  development  of  reflex 
movements  is  difficult  and  uncertain  in  the  presence  of  the  brain. 

When  we  turn  to  the  teaching  of  disease  however,  we  again 
find  that  reflex  movements  carried  out  by  the  cord  or  by  parts  of 
the  cord  are,  on  the  whole,  scanty  and  simple. 

In  some  stages  of  certain  diseases  of  the  spinal  cord  extensive 
reflex  movements  are  witnessed ;  but  these  are  not  purposeful 
coordinated  movements,  such  as  have  been  described  above  as 
occurring  in  frogs  and  mammals  after  experimental  interference, 
but  rather  mere  exaggerations  of  the  simpler  reflex  movements 
witnessed  when  the  nervous  system  is  intact,  In  cases  of  para- 
plegia (such  being  the  term  generally  used  when  disease  or  injury 
has  cut  off  the  cord,  generally  the  lower  part  of  the  cord,  from 
the  brain  so  that  the  will  cannot  bring  about  movements  in,  and 
the  mind  derives  no  sensations  from,  the  parts  below  the  lesion, 
the  legs  for  instance),  it  sometimes  happens  that  contact  with  the 
bedclothes,  or  other  external  objects,  sets  up  from  time  to  time 
rhythmically  repeated  movements,  the  legs  being  alternately 

58—2 


914  REFLEX   ACTIONS.  [BOOK  m. 

drawn  up  and  thrust  out  again.  And  an  exaggeration  of  the 
'  knee-jerk '  or  other  'tendon  reflexes '  is  a  very  common  symptom  in 
certain  spinal  diseases.  It  is  rarely  if  ever  that  reflex  movements 
of  a  really  complicated  character  are  observed.  Moreover  clinical 
experience  shews  that  in  man,  when  a  portion  of  the  cord  is 
isolated,  reflex  actions  carried  out  by  means  of  that  portion  so 
far  from  being  exaggerated  are  much  more  commonly  exceeding 
feeble  or  absent  altogether.  In  the  cases  in  which  the  physio- 
logical continuity  of  the  lower  with  the  upper  part  of  the  cord 
has  been  broken  by  disease,  by  some  growth  invading  the 
nervous  structures  or  by  some  changes  of  the  nervous  structures 
themselves,  we  may  attempt  to  explain  the  absence  from  the 
lower  part  of  coordinate  reflex  activity,  such  as  is  seen  in  the 
lower  animals,  as  due  to  the  disease  not  only  affecting  the  powers 
of  the  actually  diseased  part,  but  influencing  the  whole  cord 
below,  and  either  by  inhibition,  of  which  we  shall  speak  presently, 
or  in  some  other  way  depressing  its  functions.  But  the  same 
absence  of  complex  reflex  movements  is  also  often  observed  in 
cases  in  which  the  cord  has  been  severed  by  accident,  and  indeed, 
though  accidental  injuries  to  the  human  cord  generally  produce 
more  profound  and  extensive  mischief  than  that  which  results 
in  animals  from  skilful  experimental  interference,  clinical  ex- 
perience tends,  on  the  whole,  to  support  the  view  that  in  man 
the  more  complete  subordination  of  the  spinal  cord  to  the  brain 
has  led  to  the  dying  out  of  the  complex  reflex  actions  which 
are  so  conspicuous  in  the  lower  animals.  This  however  cannot 
be  regarded  as  distinctly  proved. 

When  we  come  to  study  voluntary  movements  we  shall  see 
reason  to  think  that  in  man,  as  in  the  lower  animals,  the  will  in 
carrying  out  these  movements  makes  use  of  complex  nervous 
mechanisms  situated  in  the  spinal  cord,  nervous  mechanisms  into 
the  working  of  which,  as  urged  above,  afferent  impulses  enter 
largely;  and  it  seems  improbable  that  these  spinal  mechanisms 
should  be  capable  of  being  thrown  into  action  by  the  will  only. 
In  the  act  of  walking  for  instance  it  is  highly  probable  that  the 
movements  of  the  legs  are  the  direct  results  of  the  action  of  nervous 
mechanisms  in  the  lumbar  cord  brought  into  play  by  the  will, 
being  thus,  in  an  indirect  manner  only,  the  products  of  volitional 
impulses ;  and  even  in  man,  though  clinical  experience  only  affords 
us  instances  of  this  machinery  working  apart  from  the  brain  in  a 
damaged  condition  and  under  unfavourable  circumstances  so  that 
the  resemblance  of  the  movements  observed  to  the  complete 
act  of  walking  is  but  feeble,  still  it  seems  similarly  probable 
that  under  more  favourable  circumstances  the  lumbar  cord  separ- 
ated from  the  brain  might  as  part  of  a  reflex  act  carry  out  the 
movements  in  a  more  complete  and  coordinate  manner. 

§  592.  We  have  dwelt  above  chiefly  on  reflex  actions,  in  which 
the  efferent  impulses  cause  contractions  of  skeletal  muscles  since 


CHAP,  i.]  THE   SPINAL   CORD.  915 

these  are  undoubtedly  the  most  common  and  the  most  prominent 
forms  of  reflex  action;  but  it  must  not  be  forgotten  that  the 
efferent  impulses  of  reflex  origin  may  produce  contractions  of 
other  muscles,  as  well  as  other  effects,  such  as  secretion  for  in- 
stance. On  several  of  these  we  have  dwelt,  from  time  to  time  in 
previous  parts  of  this  work,  and  it  will  be  unnecessary  to  repeat 
them  here.  But  it  may  be  worth  while  to  point  out  that  the 
spinal  cord  by  serving  as  a  reflex  centre  for  innumerable  ties 
which  correlate  the  nutritive  or  metabolic  activities  of  the  several 
tissues  to  events  taking  place  in  other  parts  of  the  body,  plays  a 
conspicuous  part  in  securing  the  welfare  of  the  whole  body.  In 
dealing  (§  549)  with  the  general  problems  of  nutrition,  we  stated 
that  an  orderly  nutrition  appears  to  be  in  some  way  dependent 
on  nervous  influences.  Many  of  these  nervous  influences  appear 
to  issue  from  the  spinal  cord,  either  as  parts  of  a  reflex  act,  or  as 
the  outcome  of  some  automatic  processes.  When  in  a  dog  the 
lumbar  cord  is  wholly  separated  from  the  rest  of  the  cord  by 
section,  the  nutrition  of  the  hind  limbs,  and  the  general  health 
of  the  animal  may,  with  care,  be  maintained  in  a  very  satisfactory 
condition ;  but  if  that  small  separated  piece  of  the  cord  be  des- 
troyed death  inevitably  ensues  before  long,  in  spite  of  every  care 
and  precaution,  being  brought  about  apparently  by  the  disordered 
nutrition  of  the  hind  limbs  and  other  parts  supplied  by  nerves 
coming  from  the  lumbar  cord.  In  man,  extensive  injuries  to  the 
spinal  cord  are  followed  by  bed  sores  and  other  results  of  impaired 
nutrition ;  and  indeed  death  is  generally  brought  about  in  this 
way,  in  cases  of  paraplegia  caused  by  accidental  crushing  or 
severance  of  the  cord.  The  scarcity  of  well  marked  reflex  actions 
mentioned  above  as  characteristic  of  such  cases,  may  perhaps  be 
due  to  the  fact  that  these  disorders  of  nutrition  prevent  the 
patient  living  long  enough  for  the  separated  cord  to  recover  the 
functions  which  properly  belong  to  it. 

§  593.  Inhibition  of  Reflex  Action.  The  reflex  actions  of  the 
spinal  cord,  like  other  nervous  actions,  may  be  totally  or  partially 
inhibited,  that  is  to  say  may  be  arrested  or  hindered  in  their  deve- 
lopment by  impulses  reaching  the  centre  while  it  is  already  in 
action.  Thus  if  the  body  of  a  decapitated  snake  be  allowed  to 
hang  down,  slow  rhythmic  pendulous  movements,  which  appear 
to  be  reflex  in  nature,  soon  make  their  appearance,  and  these  may 
be  for  a  while  arrested  by  slight  stimulation,  as  by  gently  stroking 
the  tail.  We  have  already  seen  that  the  action  of  such  nervous 
centres  as  the  respiratory  and  vaso-motor  centres,  which  frequently 
at  all  events  is  of  a  reflex  nature,  may  be  either  inhibited  or 
augmented  by  afferent  impulses.  The  micturition  centre  in  the 
mammal,  which  is  also  largely  a  reflex  centre,  may  be  easily  in- 
hibited by  impulses  passing  downward  to  the  lumbar  cord  from 
the  brain,  or  upward  along  the  sciatic  nerves.  In  the  case  of 
dogs,  whose  spinal  cord  has  been  divided  in  the  thoracic  region, 


916  INHIBITION   OF   REFLEX  ACTIONS.     [BOOK  in. 

micturition  set  up  as  a  reflex  act  by  simple  pressure  on  the 
abdomen  or  by  sponging  the  anus,  is  at  once  stopped  by  sharply 
pinching  the  skin  of  the  leg.  And  it  is  a  matter  of  common  ex- 
perience that  in  man  micturition  may  be  suddenly  checked  by  an 
emotion  or  other  cerebral  event.  The  erection  centre  in  the 
lumbar  cord,  also  in  large  measure  a  reflex  centre,  is  similarly 
susceptible  of  being  inhibited  by  impulses  reaching  it  from  various 
sources.  And  indeed  many  similar  instances  of  the  inhibition  of 
reflex  movements  might  readily  be  quoted. 

Several  apparent  instances  of  the  inhibition  of  reflex  acts  are 
not  really  such:  in  these  cases  all  the  nervous  processes  of  the 
act  may  take  place  in  their  entirety  and  yet  fail  to  produce  their 
effect  on  account  of  a  failure  in  the  muscular  part  of  the  act. 
Thus  when  we  ourselves  by  an  effort  of  the  will  stop  the  reflex 
movements  which  otherwise  would  be  produced  by  tickling  the 
soles  of  the  feet,  we  achieve  this  to  a  large  extent  by  throwing 
voluntarily  into  action  certain  muscles,  the  contractions  of  which 
antagonise  the  action  of  the  muscles  engaged  in  carrying  out  the 
reflex  movements.  But  it  may  be  doubted  even  in  these  cases, 
whether  inhibition  is  always  or  wholly  to  be  explained  in  this 
way ;  and  certainly  in  very  many  instances  of  reflex  inhibition, 
no  such  muscular  antagonism  is  present,  and  the  reflex  act  is 
checked  at  its  nervous  centre. 

When  the  brain  of  a  frog  is  removed,  and  the  effects  of  shock 
have  passed  away,  reflex  actions  are  developed  much  more  readily 
and  to  a  much  greater  degree  than  in  the  entire  animal,  and  in 
mammals  also  reflex  excitability  has  been  observed  to  be  increased 
by  removal  of  the  cerebral  hemispheres.  This  suggests  the  idea 
that  in  the  intact  nervous  system  the  brain  is  habitually  exerting 
some  influence  on  the  spinal  cord  tending  to  prevent  the  normal 
development  of  the  spinal  reflex  actions.  And  we  learn  by  ex- 
periment that  stimulation  of  certain  parts  of  the  brain  has  a 
remarkable  effect  on  reflex  action.  If  a  frog,  from  which  the 
cerebral  hemispheres  have  been  removed  (the  optic  lobes,  bulb 
and  spinal  cord  being  left  intact),  be  suspended  by  the  jaw,  and 
the  toes  of  the  pendent  legs  be  from  time  to  time  dipped  into  very 
dilute  sulphuric  acid,  a  certain  average  time  will  be  found  to 
elapse  between  the  dipping  of  the  toe  and  the  resulting  with- 
drawal of  the  foot.  If,  however,  the  optic  lobes  or  optic  thalami 
be  stimulated,  as  by  putting  a  crystal  of  sodium  chloride  on 
them,  it  will  be  found  on  repeating  the  experiment  while  these 
structures  are  still  under  the  influence  of  the  stimulation,  that 
the  time  intervening  between  the  action  of  the  acid  on  the  toe 
and  the  withdrawal  of  the  foot  is  very  much  prolonged.  That  is 
to  say,  the  stimulation  of  the  optic  lobes  has  caused  impulses 
to  descend  to  the  cord,  which  have  there  so  interfered  with  the 
nervous  processes  engaged  in  carrying  out  reflex  actions  as  greatly 
to  retard  the  generation  of  efferent  impulses,  or  in  other  words, 


CHAP,  i.]  THE   SPINAL   CORD.  917 

has  inhibited  the  reflex  action  of  the  cord.  And  similar  results 
may  be  obtained  in  mammals  by  stimulating  certain  parts  of 
the  corpora  quadrigemina,  which  bodies  are  homologous  to  the 
optic  lobes  of  frogs.  From  this  it  has  been  inferred  that  there 
is  present  in  this  part  of  the  brain  a  special  mechanism  for  in- 
hibiting the  reflex  actions  of  the  spinal  cord,  the  impulses 
descending  from  this  mechanism  to  the  various  centres  of  reflex 
action  being  of  a  specific  inhibitory  nature.  But,  as  we  have 
already  seen,  impulses  of  an  ordinary  kind,  passing  along  ordinary 
sensory  nerves,  may  inhibit  reflex  action.  We  have  quoted  in- 
stances where  a  slight  stimulus,  as  in  the  pendulous  movements 
of  the  snake,  and  where  *  a  stronger  stimulus  as  in  the  case 
of  the  micturition  of  the  dog,  may  produce  an  inhibitory  result ; 
we  may  add  that  in  the  frog  adequately  strong  stimuli  applied 
to  any  afferent  nerve  will  inhibit,  i.e.  will  retard  or  even  wholly 
prevent  reflex  action.  If  the  toes  of  one  leg  are  dipped  into 
dilute  sulphuric  acid  at  a  time  when  the  sciatic  of  the  other 
leg  is  being  powerfully  stimulated  with  an  interrupted  current 
the  period  of  incubation  of  the  reflex  act  will  be  found  to  be 
much  prolonged,  and  in  some  cases  the  reflex  withdrawal  of  the 
foot  will  not  take  place  at  all.  And  this  holds  good,  not  only  in 
the  complete  absence  of  the  optic  lobes  and  bulb,  but  also  when 
only  a  portion  of  the  spinal  cord,  sufficient  to  carry  out  the  reflex 
action  in  the  usual  way,  is  left.  There  can  be  no  question  here 
of  any  specific  inhibitory  centres,  such  as  have  been  supposed  to 
exist  in  the  optic  lobes.  But  if  it  is  clear  that  inhibition  of  reflex 
action  may  be  brought  about  by  impulses  which  are  not  in 
themselves  of  a  specific  inhibitory  nature,  we  may  hesitate  to 
accept  the  view  that  a  special  inhibitory  mechanism  in  the  sense 
of  one  giving  rise  to  nothing  but  inhibitory  impulses  is  present  in 
the  optic  lobes  of  frogs,  and  after  removal  of  the  brain  that  the 
exaltation  of  reflex  actions  which  is  manifest  is  due  to  the  with- 
drawal of  such  a  specific  inhibitory  mechanism. 

The  presence  of  the  brain  does  obviously  produce  an  effect., 
which  may  be  broadly  spoken  of  as  inhibitory,  and  a  specific  y 
action  of  the  brain,  in  an  effort  of  the  will,  may  stop  or  inhibit  a 
specific  reflex  action ;  but  we  must  not  in  these  matters  be  led  too 
much  away  by  the  analogy  of  the  special  and  limited  cardiac 
inhibitory  mechanism.  There  we  have  apparently  to  deal  with 
fibres,  whose  exclusive  duty  it  is  to  convey  inhibitory  impulses 
from  the  bulb  to  the  cardiac  muscle,  and  inhibition  of  the  heart, 
at  least  through  nervous  influences,  is  exclusively  carried  out  by 
them.  But  already,  in  studying  the  nervous  mechanism  of  respi- 
ration, we  have  seen  reason  to  think  that  afferent  impulses  passing 
along  the  same  nerves  and  probably  along  the  same  fibres  may, 
according  to  circumstances,  now  inhibit,  now  augment  the  respi- 
ratory centre,  and  have  thus  been  led  to  speak  of  inhibitory 
impulses,  that  is  impulses  producing  an  inhibitory  effect,  apart 


918  INHIBITION   OF   REFLEX   ACTIONS.     [BOOK  m. 

from  specific  inhibitory  fibres.  In  the  complex  working  of  the 
central  nervous  system,  we  may  still  more  expect  to  come  across 
similar  instances  of  the  same  channels  serving  as  the  path,  either 
of  inhibition  or  of  augmentation.  In  all  probability,  actions  or 
processes,  which  we  may  speak  of  as  inhibitory,  do  play,  as  indeed 
we  shall  see,  an  important  part  in  the  whole  work  of  the  central 
nervous  system ;  in  all  probability  many  of  the  phenomena  of 
nervous  life  are  the  outcome  of  a  contest  between  what  we  may 
call  inhibitory  and  exciting  or  augmenting  forces ;  but  in  all 
probability  also  we  ought  rather  to  seek  for  the  explanation  of 
how  vagus  impulses  inhibit  the  beat  of  the  heart  by  reference  to 
the  inhibitory  phenomena  of  the  central  nervous  system,  than  to 
attempt  to  explain  the  latter  by  the  little  we  know  of  the  former. 
At  present,  however,  we  must  be  content  with  the  fact  that 
experiments  on  animals  shew  that  the  brain,  not  only  by  some 
action  or  other  may  inhibit  particular  spinal  reflex  movements, 
(I  but  also  habitually  exercises  a  restraining  influence  on  the  reflex 
activity  of  the  whole  cord,  though  we  are  unable  to  state  clearly 
how  this  inhibition  is  carried  out. 

We  say  '  experiments  on  animals '  because  though  we  know,  as 
stated  above,  by  an  appeal  to  our  own  consciousness,  that  an 
action  of  the  brain,  an  effort  of  the  will,  may  stop  a  particular 
reflex  act,  we  have  no  evidence  that  in  man  separation  of  the  cord 
from  the  brain  leads,  as  in  animals,  to  heightened  reflex  activity. 
In  diseases,  or  injuries  to  the  cord,  reflex  actions  are,  as  we  have 
said,  sometimes  exaggerated,  but  it  is  possible  and  indeed  probable 
that  the  increase  is  due  to  the  morbid  processes  producing  a 
greater  irritability  of  the  cord  itself,  and  not  to  the  withdrawal  of 
any  inhibitory  influences.  In  many  cases,  in  perhaps  the  greater 
number,  no  exaggeration  but  a  diminution  or  even  absence  of 
reflex  activity  is  observed ;  so  much  so  that  could  we  trust  expli- 
citly to  clinical  experience,  we  should  be  inclined  to  conclude  that 
the  scantiness  of  spinal  reflex  action  in  man  was  due  not  to  any 
preoccupation  of  the  cord  by  influences  proceeding  from  a  dominant 
brain,  but  to  an  inherent  paucity  of  spinal  reflex  mechanisms. 
But  we  have  already  said  all  we  have  at  present  to  say  on  this 
point. 

§  594.  The  Time  required  for  Reflex  Actions.  When  one 
eyelid  is  stimulated  with  a  sharp  electrical  shock,  both  eyelids 
blink.  Hence,  if  the  length  of  time  intervening  between  the 
stimulation  of  the  right  eyelid  and  the  movement  of  the  left 
eyelid  be  measured,  this  will  give  the  total  time  required  for  the 
various  processes  which  make  up  a  reflex  action.  It  has  been 
found  to  be  from  "0662  to  '0578  sec.  Deducting  from  these  figures 
the  time  required  for  the  passage  of  afferent  and  efferent  impulses 
along  the  fifth  and  facial  nerves  to  and  from  the  bulb,  and  for  the 
latent  period  of  the  contraction  of  the  orbicularis  muscle,  there 
would  remain  '0555  to  '0471  sec.  for  the  time  consumed  in  the 


CHAP,  i.]  THE   SPINAL   CORD.  919 

central  operations  of  the  reflex  act.  The  calculations,  however, 
necessary  for  this  reduction,  it  need  not  be  said,  are  open  to 
sources  of  error ;  moreover  the  reflex  act  in  question  is  carried  out 
by  the  bulb  and  not  by  the  spinal  cord  proper.  Blinking  thus 
produced  is  a  reflex  act  of  the  very  simplest  kind ;  but  as  we  have 
seen  in  the  preceding  pages,  reflex  acts  differ  very  widely  in  nature 
and  character;  and  we  accordingly  find,  as  indeed  we  have 
incidentally  mentioned,  that  the  time  taken  up  by  a  reflex 
movement  varies  very  largely.  This  indeed  is  seen  in  blinking 
itself.  When  the  blinking  is  caused  not  by  an  electric  shock 
applied  to  the  eyelid,  but  by  a  flash  of  light  falling  on  the  retina, 
in  which  case  complex  visual  processes  are  involved,  the  time 
is  distinctly  prolonged ;  moreover  the  results  in  different  ex- 
periments in  which  light  serves  as  the  stimulus  are  not  nearly  so 
uniform  as  when  the  blinking  is  caused  by  stimulation  of  the 
eyelid. 

In  general  it  may  be  said  that  the  time  required  for  any 
reflex  act  varies  very  considerably  with  the  strength  of  the 
stimulus  employed,  being  less  for  the  stronger  stimuli;  this  we 
should  expect,  seeing  that  the  efferent  impulses  of  the  reflex  act 
are  not  simply  afferent  impulses  transmitted  through  the  central 
organ,  but  result  from  internal  changes  in  the  central  organ  started 
by  the  afferent  impulse  or  impulses;  and  these  internal  changes 
will  naturally  be  more  intense  and  more  rapidly  effected  when  the 
afferent  impulses  are  strong.  It  is  stated  that  when  the  movement 
induced  is  on  the  same  side  of  the  body  as  the  surface  stimulation 
of  which  starts  the  act,  the  time  taken  up  is  less  than  when  the 
movement  is  on  the  other  side  of  the  body,  allowance  being  made 
for  the  length  of  central  nervous  matter  involved  in  the  two  cases ; 
that  is  to  say  the  central  operations  of  a  reflex  act  are  propagated 
more  rapidly  along  the  cord  than  across  the  cord.  The  rapidity 
of  the  act  varies  of  course  with  the  condition  of  the  spinal 
cord,  the  act  being  greatly  prolonged  when  the  cord  becomes 
exhausted ;  and  a  similar  delay  has  been  observed  in  cases  of 
disease.  The  time  thus  occupied  by  purely  reflex  actions  must 
not  be  confounded  with  the  interval  required  when  the  changes 
taking  place  in  the  central  nervous  system  are  of  a  more  compli- 
cated nature,  and  more  or  less  distinctly  involve  mental  operations  ; 
of  the  latter  we  shall  speak  later  on. 


SEC.   4.     THE   AUTOMATIC   ACTIONS   OF   THE 
SPINAL    CORD. 


§  595.  We  speak  of  an  action  of  an  organ  or  of  a  living  body 
as  being  spontaneous  or  automatic  when  it  appears  to  be  not 
immediately  due  to  any  changes  in  the  circumstances  in  which  the 
organ  or  body  is  placed,  but  to  be  the  result  of  changes  arising  in 
the  organ  or  body  itself  and  determined  by  causes  other  than  the 
influences  of  the  circumstances  of  the  moment.  Some  automatic 
actions  are  of  a  continued  character ;  others,  like  the  beat  of  the 
heart,  are  repeated  in  regular  rhythm ;  but  the  most  striking 
automatic  actions  of  the  living  body,  those  which  we  attribute  to 
the  working  of  the  will  and  which  we  call  voluntary  or  volitional, 
are  characterized*  by  their  apparent  irregularity  and  variableness. 
Such  variable  automatic  actions  form  the  most  striking  features 
of  an  intact  nervous  system,  but  are  conspicuously  absent  from  a 
spinal  cord  when  the  brain  has  been  removed. 

A  brainless  frog  placed  in  a  condition  of  complete  equilibrium 
in  which  no  stimulus  is  brought  to  bear  on  it,  protected  for  in- 
stance from  sudden  passing  changes  in  temperature,  from  a  too 
rapid  evaporation  by  the  skin  and  the  like,  remains  perfectly 
motionless  until  it  dies.  Such  apparently  spontaneous  movements 
as  are  occasionally  witnessed  are  so  few  and  seldom,  that  we  can 
hardly  do  otherwise  than  attribute  them  to  some  stimulus,  internal 
or  external,  which  has  escaped  observation.  In  the  mammal  (dog) 
after  division  of  the  spinal  cord  in  the  dorsal  region  regular  and 
apparently  spontaneous  movements  may  be  observed  in  the  parts 
governed  by  the  lumbar  cord.  When  the  animal  has  thoroughly 
recovered  from  the  operation  the  hind  limbs  rarely  remain  quiet 
for  any  long  period ;  they  move  restlessly  in  various  ways ;  and 
when  the  animal  is  suspended  by  the  upper  part  of  the  body,  the 
pendent  hind  limbs  are  continually  being  drawn  up  and  let  down 
again  with  a  monotonous  rhythmic  regularity,  suggestive  of 
automatic  rhythmic  discharges  from  the  central  mechanisms  of 
the  cord.  In  the  newly  born  mammal  too,  after  removal  of  the 


CHAP,  i.]  THE   SPINAL   CORD.  921 

brain,  movements  apparently  spontaneous  in  nature  are  frequently 
observed.  But  all  these  movements,  even  when  most  highly  deve- 
loped, are  very  different  from  the  movements,  irregular  and  variable 
in  their  occurrence  though  orderly  and  purposeful  in  their  character, 
which  we  recognize  as  distinctly  voluntary.  Even  admitting  that 
some  of  the  movements  of  the  brainless  mammal  may  resemble 
voluntary  movements  in  so  far  as  they  are  due  to  changes  taking 
place  in  the  spinal  cord  itself  independent  of  the  immediate 
influence  of  any  stimulus,  we  are  not  thereby  justified  in  speaking 
of  the  spinal  cord  as  developing  a  will  in  the  sense  that  we 
attribute  a  will  to  the  brain. 

§  596.  In  the  case  of  the  beat  of  the  heart,  the  automatic 
rhythmic  discharge  of  energy  appears  to  be  exclusively  the  outcome 
of  the  molecular  nutritive  changes  taking  place  in  the  cardiac 
substance.  The  beat  may  be  modified,  as  we  have  seen,  by  nervous 
impulses  reaching  the  cardiac  substance  along  certain  nerves; 
but  the  actual  existence  of  the  beat  is  wholly  independent  of  these 
extraneous  influences ;  the  rhythmic  discharge  continues  when  they 
.are  entirely  absent.  The  automatic  rhythmic  discharge  of  respi- 
ratory impulses  from  the  respiratory  centre  is  also  dependent  on 
the  intrinsic  molecular  changes  of  the  centre,  these  being,  as  we 
have  seen,  largely  determined  by  the  character  of  the  blood 
streaming  through  it ;  but  in  this  case  extrinsic  nervous  impulses, 
reaching  the  centre  along  the  vagus  and  other  nerves,  play  a  much 
more  important  part  than  do  similar  impulses  in  the  case  of  the 
heart.  They  act  so  continually  on  the  centre  and  enter  so  largely 
into  its  working,  that  we  are  compelled  to  regard  the  activity  of 
the  centre  as  fed,  if  we  may  use  the  word,  not  only  by  the 
intrinsic  molecular  nutritive  processes  of  the  centre  itself,  but  also 
by  the  extrinsic  nervous  influences  which  flow  into  the  centre  from 
without.  The  automatism  of  the  spinal  cord  as  a  whole  resembles, 
in  this  aspect,  that  of  the  respiratory  centre  rather  than  that  of 
the  heart.  It  has  for  its  basis  doubtless  the  intrinsic  molecular 
changes  of  the  grey  matter,  on  whose  remarkable  constitution  we 
dwelt  in  a  previous  section  ;  the  metabolic  events  of  this  substance 
are  so  ordered  as  to  give  rise  to  discharges  of  energy;  but  the 
discharge  appears  to  be  also  intimately  dependent  on  the  inflow 
into  the  grey  matter  of  afferent  impulses  and  influences.  The 
normal  discharge  of  efferent  impulses  from  the  cord  undoubtedly 
takes  place  under  the  influence  of  these  incoming  impulses ;  and 
it  may  be  doubted  whether  the  grey  matter  of  the  cord  would  be 
able,  in  the  absence  of  all  afferent  impulses,  to  generate  any  sus- 
tained series  of  discharges  out  of  its  merely  nutritive  intrinsic 
changes.  The  automatic  activity  of  the  cord  is  fed  not  only  by 
intrinsic  nutritive  events,  but  also  by  extrinsic  influences. 

In  this  feature  we  may,  moreover,  find  perhaps  the  reason  why 
the  automatic  activity  of  the  spinal  cord  is  so  limited,  as  compared 
with  that  of  the  brain.  In  spite  of  certain  striking  but  superficial 


922  TONE   OF   SKELETAL   MUSCLES.          [BOOK  in. 

characters  of  which  we  shall  speak  later  on,  the  grey  matter  of  the 
brain  presents  no  histological  features  so  different  from  those  of 
the  grey  matter  of  the  cord,  as  to  justify  us  in  concluding  that  the 
one  is  capable  and  the  other  incapable  of  developing  the  impulses, 
which  we  call  volitional,  out  of  the  molecular  nutritive  changes  of 
its  substance.  We  are,  therefore,  led  to  the  conclusion  that  the 
fuller  automatic  activity  of  the  brain  is  due  to  the  intrinsic 
changes  of  its  substance  being  so  much  more  largely  assisted  by 
the  influx  of  various  afferent  impulses  and  influences,  notably 
those  of  the  special  senses.  To  this  question,  however,  we  shall 
have  to  return  later  on. 

§  597.  In  treating  of  the  vascular  system  we  saw  that  the 
central  nervous  system  exercised  through  the  vaso-motor  nerves 
such  an  influence  on  the  muscular  coats  of  the  blood  vessels  as  to 
maintain,  what  we  spoke  of  as  '  tone,'  section  of  vaso-constrictor 
fibres  leading  to  "  loss  of  tone."  We  saw  further,  that  arterial 
tone,  though  normally  dependent  on  the  general  vaso-motor  centre 
in  the  bulb,  could  be  kept  up  by  the  cord  itself,  that  for  instance 
a  tone  of  the  blood  vessels  of  the  hind-limbs  could  be  maintained 
by  the  isolated  dorso-lumbar  cord.  This  maintenance  of  arterial 
tone  may  be  spoken  of  as  one  of  the  "  automatic  "  functions  of  the 
spinal  cord.  We  have  also  seen  that  plain  muscular  fibres,  other 
than  those  of  the  arteries,  notably  the  fibres  forming  sphincters, 
such  as  the  cardiac  and  pyloric  sphincters  of  the  stomach,  the 
sphincter  of  the  bladder,  and  especially  the  sphincter  of  the  anus, 
also  possess  tone,  and  that  the  tone  of  these  sphincters  is  also 
dependent  on  the  spinal  cord,  or  on  some  part  of  the  central 
nervous  system.  We  need  not  repeat  the  discussions  concerning 
these  mechanisms  and  other  instances  of  the  spinal  cord  exer- 
cising an  automatic  influence  over  various  viscera ;  we  have 
referred  to  them  here,  since  they  serve  as  an  introduction  to  a 
question  which  has  been  much  debated,  and  which  has  many 
collateral  and  important  bearings,  namely  the  question  whether 
the  spinal  cord  exercises  an  automatic  function  in  maintaining  a 
tone  of  the  skeletal  muscles. 

The  question  is  not  one  which,  like  the  case  of  arterial  tone, 
can  be  settled  off  hand  by  a  simple  experiment.  Most  observers 
agree  that  the  section  of  a  motor  nerve  does  not  produce  any 
clearly  recognizable  immediate  lengthening  of  a  muscle  supplied 
by  the  nerve,  in  the  same  way  that  section  of  a  vaso-constrictor 
nerve  undoubtedly  gives  rise  to  a  relaxation  of  the  muscular  fibres 
in  the  arteries  governed  by  it ;  and  it  has  been  inferred  from  this 
that  skeletal  tone  does  not  exist.  But  there  are  several  facts 
to  be  taken  into  consideration  before  we  can  come  to  a  just 
decision. 

The  skeletal  muscles  have  been  described  as  being  placed  "  on 
the  stretch  "  in  the  living  body.  If  a  muscle  be  cut  away  from  its 
attachments  at  each  end,  it  shortens ;  if  it  be  cut  across,  it  gapes. 


CHAP,  i.]  THE   SPINAL   CORD.  923 

In  other  words,  the  muscle  in  the  living  body  possesses  a  latent 
tendency  to  shorten,  which  is  continually  being  counteracted  by 
its  disposition  and  attachments.  In  studying  muscular  contraction 
we  saw  (§  87)  that  the  shortening  of  a  contraction  is  followed  by  a 
relaxation  or  return  to  the  former  length,  both  the  contraction 
and  relaxation  being  the  result  of  molecular  changes  in  the  living 
muscular  substance.  We  have  now  to  extend  our  view  and  to 
recognize  that,  apart  from  the  occurrence  of  ordinary  contractions, 
molecular  changes  are  by  means  of  nutritive  processes  continually 
going  on  in  the  muscle  in  such  a  way  that  the  muscle,  though 
continually  on  the  stretch,  does  not  permanently  lengthen,  but 
retains  the  power  to  shorten  upon  removal  or  lessening  of  the 
stretch,  and  conversely  though  possessing  this  power  of  shortening 
permits  itself  to  lengthen  when  the  stretch  is  increased.  In  this 
way  the  muscle  is  able  to  accommodate  itself  to  variations  in  the 
amount  of  stretch  to  which  it  is  from  time  to  time  subjected. 
When  a  flexor  muscle  for  instance  contracts,  the  antagonistic 
extensor  muscle  is  put  on  an  increased  stretch  and  is  corre- 
spondingly lengthened ;  when  the  contraction  of  the  flexor  passes 
off  the  extensor  returns  to  its  previous  length ;  and  so  in  other 
instances.  Thus  by  virtue  of  certain  changes  within  itself  a 
muscle  maintains  what  may  be  called  its  natural  length  in  the 
body,  always  returning  to  that  natural  length  both  after  being 
shortened  and  after  being  stretched.  In  this  the  muscle  does  no 
more  than  do  the  other  tissues  of  the  body  which,  within  limits, 
retain  their  natural  form  under  the  varied  stress  and  strain  of  life ; 
but  the  property  is  conspicuous  in  the  muscle ;  and  its  effects  in 
skeletal  muscles  correspond  so  closely  to  those  of  arterial  tone, 
that  we  may  venture  to  speak  of  it  as  skeletal  tone.  Indeed,  the 
molecular  changes  at  the  bottom  of  both  are  probably  the  same. 

These  changes  are  an  expression  of  the  life  of  the  muscle; 
they  disappear  when  the  muscle  dies  and  enters  into  rigor  mortis ; 
and  moreover,  during  life  they  vary  in  intensity  so  that  the  '  tone ' 
varies  in  amount  according  to  the  nutritive  changes  going  on. 
We  have  seen  reason  to  believe  that  the  nutrition  of  a  muscle  as 
of  other  tissues  is  governed  in  some  way  by  the  central  nervous 
system.  We  saw,  in  treating  of  muscle  and  nerve  (§  83),  that 
the  irritability  of  a  muscle  is  markedly  affected  by  the  section  of 
its  nerve,  i.e.  by  severance  from  the  central  nervous  system ;  and 
again  (§  549)  in  speaking  of  the  so-called  trophic  action  of  the 
nervous  system,  we  referred  to  changes  in  the  nutrition  of  muscles 
occasioned  by  diseases  of  the  nervous  system.  And  experience, 
especially  clinical  experience,  shews  that  the  nutritive  changes 
which  determine  tone  are  very  closely  dependent  on  a  due  action 
of  the  central  nervous  .system.  When  we  handle  the  limb  of  a 
healthy  man,  we  find  that  it  offers  a  certain  amount  of  resistance 
to  passive  movements.  This  resistance,  which  is  quite  indepen- 
dent of,  that  is  to  say,  which  may  be  clearly  recognized  in  the 


924  TONE   OF   SKELETAL   MUSCLES.          [Boon  m. 

absence  of  all  distinct  muscular  contractions  of  volitional  or  other 
origin,  is  an  expression  of  muscular  tone,  of  the  effort  of  the 
various  muscles  to  maintain  their  'natural'  length.  In  many 
cases  of  disease  this  resistance  is  felt  to  be  obviously  less  than 
normal;  the  limb  is  spoken  of  as  "  limp  "  or  "  flabby;"  or  as  having 
*a  want  of  tone.'  In  other  cases  of  disease,  on  the  other  hand,  this 
resistance  is  markedly  increased ;  the  limb  is  felt  to  be  stiff  or 
rigid ;  more  or  less  force  is  needed  to  change  it  from  a  flexed  to  an 
extended,  or  from  an  extended  to  a  flexed  condition ;  and,  in  the 
range  of  disease,  we  may  meet  with  very  varying  amounts  of 
increased  resistance,  from  a  condition  which  is  only  slightly  above 
the  normal  to  one  of  extreme  rigidity.  In  some  cases  the  condition 
of  the  muscle  is  such  as  at  first  sight  seems  much  more  comparable 
to  a  permanent  ordinary  contraction  than  to  a  mere  exaggeration 
of  normal  tone ;  but  all  intermediate  stages  are  met  with ;  and 
indeed  these  extreme  cases  may  be  taken  as  indicating  that  the 
molecular  processes  which  maintain  what  we  are  now  calling  tone, 
are  at  bottom,  of  the  same  nature  as  those  which  carry  out  a 
contraction ;  they  serve  to  shew  the  fundamental  identity  of  the 
skeletal  tone  with  the  more  obvious  arterial  tone. 

Clinical  experience  then  shews  that  the  central  nervous  system 
does  exert  on  the  skeletal  muscles  such  an  influence  as  to  give  rise 
to  what  we  may  speak  of  as  skeletal  tone,  changes  in  the  central 
nervous  system,  leading  in  some  cases  to  diminution  or  loss  of  tone, 
in  other  cases  to  exaggeration  of  tone,  manifested  often  as  con- 
spicuous rigidity.  The  question  why  the  changes  take  one 
direction  in  one  case  and  another  in  another  is  one  of  great 
difficulty  (the  occurrence  of  extreme  rigidity  being  especially 
obscure),  and  cannot  be  discussed  here.  We  have  called  attention 
to  the  facts  simply  because  they  shew  the  existence  of  skeletal 
tone  and  its  dependence  on  the  central  nervous  system.  This 
conclusion  is  confirmed  by  experiments  on  animals,  and  these  also 
afford  proof  that  in  animals  the  spinal  cord  can  by  itself,  apart 
from  the  brain,  maintain  the  existence  of  such  a  tone.  In  a  frog, 
after  division  of  the  cord  below  the  brain,  the  limbs  during  the 
period  of  shock  are  flabby  and  toneless ;  but  after  a  while,  as  the 
shock  passes  off,  tone  returns  to  the  muscles,  and  the  limbs  offer 
when  handled  a  resistance  like  that  of  the  limbs  of  an  entire  frog. 
When  the  animal  is  suspended  the  hind  limbs  do  not  hang 
perfectly  limp  and  helpless,  but  assume  a  definite  position ;  and 
that  this  position  is  due  to  some  influence  proceeding  from  the 
spinal  cord  is  shewn  by  dividing  the  sciatic  nerve  on  one  side ;  the 
hind  limb  on  that  side  now  hangs  quite  helpless.  This  more 
pendent  position  shews  that  some  of  the  flexors  have  lengthened 
in  consequence  of  the  section  of  the  nerve,  and  this  result  may  be 
taken  as  refuting  the  argument,  quoted  above  against  the  existence 
of  tone,  which  is  based  on  the  statement  that  a  muscle  cannot  be 
observed  to  lengthen  after  section  of  its  nerve.  It  may  be  here 


CHAP,  i.]  THE   SPINAL   CORD.  925 

remarked  that  if  the  brainless  frog,  whose  hind-limbs  are  more  or 
less  pendent  when  the  body  is  suspended,  be  placed  on  its  belly 
the  hind-limbs  are  brought  into  a  flexed  position  under  the  body 
by  means  of  obvious  muscular  contraction  ;  and  from  this  it  might 
be  inferred  that  the  maintenance  of  the  position  of  the  pendent 
limb  was  also  the  result  of  a  feeble  contraction.  But  no  obvious 
contractions  can  be  observed  in  the  latter  case,  as  in  the  former ; 
and  when  in  the  former  the  limb  has  once  been  brought  into  the 
flexed  position,  that  position,  like  the  pendent  position,  is  main- 
tained without  obvious  contractions.  As  we  said  above  'tone' 
may  pass  into  something  which  appears  to  be  identical  with  a 
contraction,  but  where  no  obvious  contractions  are  observed  it 
seems  preferable  to  speak  of  the  state  of  the  muscle  as  one  of 
tone. 

In  the  dog,  after  division  of  the  cord  in  the  thoracic  region,  the 
hind-limbs  during  the  period  of  shock  are  limp  and  toneless.  In 
the  warm  blooded  animal,  as  we  have  said,  the  effects  of  shock  are 
much  more  lasting  than  in  the  cold  blooded  animal ;  and  in  the 
dog  the  tone  of  the  skeletal  muscle  returns  much  more  slowly  than 
in  the  frog.  Indeed  when  the  division  of  the  cord  has  taken  place 
low  down  the  skeletal  tone  returns  very  slowly,  and  may  be  mani- 
fested very  feebly,  or  even  be  absent  altogether.  But  under 
favourable  circumstances,  when  a  sufficient  length  of  cord  has  been 
left,  a  fairly  normal  tone  is  reestablished.  In  man,  in  accordance 
with  the  facts  previously  mentioned  (§591)  skeletal  tone,  which  has 
been  lost  through  the  continuity  of  the  cord  being  broken  by 
disease  or  accident,  appears  rarely  if  ever  to  return  fully  in  the 
regions  below  the  lesion. 

We  may  therefore  on  the  whole  of  the  evidence  conclude  that 
the  maintenance  of  skeletal  tone  is  one  of  the  functions  of  the  cord ; 
but  we  may  here  repeat  that  the  condition  of  the  cord,  on  which 
depends  the  issue  from  the  cord  along  efferent  nerves  of  the 
influences,  whatever  their  nature,  which  produce  tone  in  the 
muscle,  may  be,  and  indeed  is,  in  its  turn  dependent  on  afferent 
impulses.  In  the  case  of  the  frog  quoted  above  the  tone  of  the 
pendent  limbs  disappears  or  is  greatly  lessened  when  the  posterior 
roots  of  the  sciatic  nerves  are  divided,  though  the  anterior  roots  be 
left  intact.  In  the  absence  of  the  usual  stream  of  afferent  impulses 
passing  into  it,  the  cord  ceases  to  send  forth  the  influences  which 
maintain  the  tone.  Hence  the  maintenance  of  tone  presents  many 
analogies  with  a  reflex  action  especially  when  we  remember  that, 
as  stated  above,  tone  passes  insensibly  into  contraction;  and  it  may 
seem  a  mere  matter  of  words  whether  we  speak  of  the  maintenance 
of  tone  as  an  automatic  or  as  a  reflex  action  of  the  cord.  We  may, 
however,  distinguish  the  part  played  by  the  afferent  impulses  in 
assisting  the  cord  to  a  condition  in  which  it  is  capable  of 
maintaining  tone  from  the  part  played  by  an  afferent  impulse  in 
causing  a  reflex  action;  in  the  former  the  action  of  the  afferent 


926  KNEE-JERK.  [BOOK  in. 

impulses  seems  analogous  to  that  of  a  supply  of  arterial  blood  in 
maintaining  an  adequate  irritability  of  the  nervous  substance,  in 
the  latter  the  afferent  impulses  lead  directly  to  a  discharge  of 
energy.  And  it  is  convenient  to  distinguish  the  two  things  by 
different  names. 

§  598.  The  close  connection  between  tone  and  reflex  action  is 
illustrated  by  the  so-called  '  tendon-phenomena,'  which,  on  the  one 
hand,  are  considered  as  cases  of  ordinary  reflex  action,  and,  on  the 
other  hand,  have  been  regarded  as  exemplifying  a  special  influence 
of  the  spinal  cord  on  the  irritability  of  the  muscles.  It  is  well 
known  that  when  the  leg  is  placed  in  an  easy  position,  resting  for 
instance  on  the  other  leg,  a  sharp  blow  on  the  patellar  tendon  will 
cause  a  sudden  jerk  forward  of  the  leg,  brought  about  by  a 
contraction  of  the  quadriceps  femoris ;  it  is  necessary  or  at  least 
desirable  for  a  good  development  of  the  jerk  that  the  tendon  (and 
muscle)  should  be  somewhat  on  the  stretch.  Similarly  the  muscles 
of  the  calf  may  be  thrown  into  action  by  tapping  the  tendo 
Achillis  put  somewhat  on  the  stretch  by  flexion  of  the  foot ;  and 
in  some  cases  the  same  muscles  may  be  made  to  execute  a  series 
of  regular  rhythmic  contractions,  called  'clonic'  contractions,  by 
suddenly  pressing  back  the  sole  of  the  foot  so  as  to  put  them  on 
the  stretch.  These,  and  other  instances  of  a  like  kind,  at  first 
sight  appear  to  be,  and  indeed  are  by  many  observers  maintained 
to  be,  cases  of  reflex  action,  due  to  afferent  impulses  started  in  the 
tendon;  hence  they  have  been  frequently  spoken  of  as  'tendon- 
reflex.'  Other  observers  maintain  that  they  are  not  reflex,  but 
due  to  direct  stimulation  of  the  muscles,  the  vibrations  set  up  in 
the  more  or  less  tense  tendon  being  transmitted  to  the  muscles 
and  so  throwing  the  latter  into  contractions.  The  chief  arguments 
against  their  being  reflex  are  that  the  interval  between  the  tap 
and  the  contraction  is  very  short  '03  or  '04  sec.,  shorter  than  the 
ordinary  interval  of  a  reflex  action  (§  594)  and  that  the  movement 
persists  after  section  of  the  nerves  of  the  tendon.  The  first 
argument  is  perhaps  not  a  very  strong  one,  and  the  second  may  be 
met  by  supposing  that,  in  such  a  case  at  least,  if  not  always,  the 
reflex  act  really  begins  in  the  muscle  being  started  in  it  by  the 
vibrations  transmitted  to  it  along  the  tendon. 

But  even  if  we  admit  that  the  movements  are  purely  muscular, 
started  and  carried  out  in  the  muscle  without  the  help  of  the  usual 
reflex  chain  of  afferent  impulses,  spinal  centre  and  efferent  im- 
pulses, we  must  at  the  same  time  admit  that  they  are  closely 
dependent  on  the  integrity  of  the  spinal  cord  and  of  the  connec- 
tions between  the  cord  and  the  muscle.  In  the  case  of  animals 
they  disappear  when  the  spinal  cord  is  destroyed,  or  the  nerves 
going  to  the  muscles  are  severed,  or  even  when  the  posterior  roots 
only  are  divided.  The  measure  of  their  development  both  in 
animals  and  in  man  is  also  closely  dependent  on  the  condition  of  the 
spinal  cord  and  of  the  central  nervous  system  generally.  They  may 


CHAP,  i.]  THE   SPINAL   CORD.  927 

be  increased  or  diminished,  augmented  or  inhibited  by  a  coincident 
voluntary  effort  directed  towards  some  other  end,  or  by  the  coinci- 
dent development  of  a  sufficiently  distinct  sensation.  In  general 
it  may  be  said  that  whatever  favours  the  activity  of  the  spinal 
cord  tends  to  increase  them,  and  whatever  depresses  the  activity 
of  the  spinal  cord  tends  to  diminish  them.  They  are  diminished 
or  wanting  in  certain  diseases  of  the  spinal  cord  (e.g.  locomotor 
ataxy),  and  exaggerated  in  others ;  so  much  so  indeed  that  they 
have  become  of  practical  clinical  importance  as  a  means  of 
diagnosis.  Whether  we  regard  them  as  instances  of  ordinary 
reflex  action,  or  consider  that  they  are  carried  out  by  the  muscle 
itself  and  that  the  cord  intervenes  only  so  far  as  to  increase, 
maintain  or  diminish  the  irritability  of  the  muscular  substance,  it 
remains  good  that  they  are  prominent  whenever  the  conditions 
increase  the  reflex  or  other  excitability  of  the  cord,  and  diminish 
or  disappear  when  the  conditions  lower  or  abolish  that  excita- 
bility. 

§  599.  Disease  in  man  reveals  other  actions  of  the  spinal  cord 
which  bear  features  different  from  those  of  an  ordinary  reflex 
movement,  and  yet  have  been  described  as  reflex  in  nature.  For 
instance  certain  affections  of  the  cord  are  characterized  by  the 
legs  becoming  rigid  in  extreme  extension,  the  rigidity  of  the 
straightened  limbs  being  often  so  great,  that  when  a  bystander 
lifts  up  one  leg  from  the  bed,  the  other  leg  is  raised  at  the  same 
time.  The  rigidity  is  due  to  the  extensor  muscles  being  thrown 
into  a  state  of  contraction,  which  is  so  uniform  and  long  con- 
tinued that  it  may  be  spoken  of  as  a  "tonic"  contraction;  such 
a  tonic  rigidity  may  however  be  replaced  by  a  series  of  rhythmic 
"  clonic,"  contractions.  It  has  sometimes  been  observed  that  the 
limbs  when  flexed  are  supple  and  free  from  rigidity,  but  that 
rigidity  sets  in  so  soon  as  they  are  brought  into  the  position  of 
extension,  the  leg  becoming  suddenly  fixed  and  straight  somewhat 
in  the  way  that  a  clasp-knife  springs  back  when  opened.  It 
seems  clear  that  the  peculiar  contraction  is  carried  out  by 
means  of  the  spinal  cord,  but  the  whole  action,  though  it  is 
often  spoken  of  as  a  'muscle-reflex/  is  very  unlike  an  ordinary 
reflex  movement.  In  an  ordinary  movement  an  extensor  is 
brought  into  action  when  a  limb  is  flexed,  not  when  it  is  already 
extended ;  and  if  in  a  reflex  act  the  condition  of  the  muscle 
about  to  be  thrown  into  action  determines  in  any  way  the 
discharge  of  impulses  from  the  reflex  centre,  we  should  expect 
that  the  stretching  of  an  extensor  muscle  by  flexion,  not  its 
relaxation  by  extension,  would  determine  the  discharge  of 
extensor  impulses.  In  the  case  of  the  diseases  in  question  just 
the  opposite  seems  to  take  place ;  the  position  which  appears  to 
determine  the  development  of  the  remarkable  contraction  is 
precisely  that  in  which  the  strain  upon  the  extensors  is  at  its 
minimum.  It  may  be  doubted,  therefore,  whether  the  word 

F.  59 


928  SPASMODIC   RIGIDITY.  [BOOK  m. 

reflex  should  be  used  to  denote  such  phenomena ;  but  the  pheno- 
mena themselves  deserve  attention,  especially  perhaps  as  shewing 
how  in  the  disorders  of  the  grey  matter  of  the  cord  due  to 
disease  impulses  or  influences  which  are  latent  only  in  health 
become  actual  and  effective. 

It  remains  for  us  to  speak  of  the  part  played  by  the  spinal 
cord,  as  the  instrument  of  the  brain,  in  the  execution  of  voluntary 
movements  and  in  the  development  of  conscious  sensations ;  but  it 
will  be  best  to  consider  these  matters  in  connection  with  the  brain 
itself,  to  the  study  of  which  we  must  now  turn. 


CHAPTER   II. 
THE   BRAIN. 

SEC.    1.     ON   SOME   GENERAL   FEATURES   OF   THE 
STRUCTURE   OF   THE   BRAIN. 


§  600.  It  would  be  out  of  place  to  attempt  to  give  here  a 
complete  description  of  the  structure  of  the  brain ;  but  certain 
features  must  be  kept  fresh  in  the  mind  as  a  basis  for  physiological 
discussion ;  and  to  these  we  must  now  turn  our  attention,  a 
general  acquaintance  with  the  topographical  anatomy  of  the  brain 
being  presupposed1. 

Like  the  spinal  cord  the  brain  consists  of  4  white  matter,'  in 
which  the  nervous  elements  are  almost  exclusively  medullated 
fibres,  and  of  '  grey  matter/  in  which  nerve-cells  and  other  nervous 
elements  are  also  present ;  but  the  grey  matter  of  the  brain  is 
much  more  variable  in  structure  than  that  of  the  spinal  cord,  and 
possesses  features  peculiar  to  itself;  these  we  shall  study  later  on. 

For  physiological  purposes  the  brain  may  be  conveniently  di- 
vided into  parts  corresponding  to  the  divisions  which  appear  in  it  in 
the  embryo.  At  an  early  stage  in  the  life  of  the  embryo,  that  part  of 
the  medullary  tube  which  is  about  to  become  the  brain  differs  from 
that  which  is  about  to  become  the  spinal  cord,  in  that  the  central 
canal,  which  in  the  latter  is  of  fairly  uniform  bore  along  its  whole 
length,  is  in  the  former  alternately  widened  and  narrowed,  so  that 
the  tube  forms  a  series  of  vesicles,  the  cerebral  vesicles,  succeeding 
each  other  lengthways.  At  first  these  vesicles  are  three  in 
number,  called  respectively  fore-brain,  mid-brain,  and  hind-brain ; 
but  the  fore-brain  after  having  developed  on  each  side  a  lateral 
vesicle,  the  optic  vesicle,  subsequently  transformed  into  the  retina 

1  Figs.  108  to  123  which  will  be  found  in  succeeding  sections  may  with 
advantage  be  consulted  in  reading  this  section  though  not  specially  referred  to  in 
the  text. 

59—2 


930  GENERAL   STRUCTURE.  [BOOK  in. 

and  optic  nerve,  gives  rise  in  front  of  itself  to  a  pair  of  vesicles 
placea  side  by  side,  or  rather  to  a  single  vesicle  with  a  deep 
median  furrow,  the  vesicle  of  the  cerebrum,  containing  a  cavity 
divided  by  a  median  partition  into  two  cavities,  lying  side  by 
side,  which  open  into  the  cavity  of  the  original  fore-brain 
by  a  Y-shaped  opening.  This  embryonic  chain  of  vesicles  is 
developed  into  the  adult  brain  by  unequal  growth  of  the  walls 
and  unequal  expansion  of  the  cavities,  certain  features  being  also 
impressed  upon  it  by  the  bend  on  the  longitudinal  axis,  which 
takes  place  in  the  region  of  the  mid-brain  and  is  known  as  the 
cranial  flexure. 

§  601.  In  the  hind  part  of  the  hinder  vesicle  or  hind-brain, 
the  ventral,  basal  portion  or  floor  is  thickened  to  form  the  bulb, 
while  the  greater  part  of  the  dorsal  portion  or  roof  does  not  thicken 
at  all,  is  not  transformed  into  nervous  elements,  but  remains  as 
a  single  layer  of  epithelium,  adherent  to  the  pia  mater  overlying 
it,  and  so  forms  a  thin  covering  to  the  lozenge-shaped  cavity  of 
the  vesicle,  now  known  as  the  fourth  ventricle. 

In  the  front  part  of  the  same  hind-brain,  on  the  contrary,  the 
roof  and  sides  are  enormously  developed  into  the  conspicuous 
cerebellum  overhanging  the  front  part  of  the  fourth  ventricle, 
while  the  floor  is  also  thickened  into  the  pons  Varolii. 

This  thickening  of  the  pons  is  largely  made  up  on  the  one 
hand  of  horizontal  nerve  fibres,  which  run  transversely  from  each 
side  of  the  cerebellum  into  the  pons  or  from  one  side  of  the 
cerebellum  to  the  other,  and  on  the  other  hand  of  longitudinal 
.fibres,  which  run  forwards  from  the  bulb  and  are  wrapped 
round  by  and  interlaced  with  the  others.  At  the  front  margin  of 
the  pons  thes'e  longitudinal  fibres,  augmented  in  number,  appear  as 
two  thick  strands,  the  crura  cerebri,  forming  the  floor  of  the 
mid-brain,  the  roof  of  which  is  thickened  into  the  corpora  quadri- 
gemina,  and  the  cavity  of  which  is  reduced  to  a  narrow  tubular 
passage,  the  aqueduct  of  Sylvius,  or  Her  a  tertio  ad  quartum 
ventriculum. 

At  the  level  of  the  fore-brain  the  crura  cerebri,  diverging 
rapidly  from  each  other  as  they  pass  forwards,  leave  the  median 
portion  of  the  floor  of  the  vesicle  now  known  as  the  third  ventricle 
very  thin,  but  form,  especially  behind  and  ventrally,  thick  lateral 
walls,  which  are  further  increased  in  thickness  by  the  development 
on  each  side  of  a  mass  largely  composed  of  grey  matter,  known  as 
the  optic  thalamus.  The  roof  of  the  third  ventricle,  like  that  of 
the  fourth  ventricle,  is  not  developed  into  nervous  elements  but 
remains  extremely  thin,  and  consists  of  nothing  more  than  a 
single  layer  of  epithelium. 

§  602.  In  front  of  the  third  ventricle  each  diverging  crus 
cerebri  spreads  out  in  a  radial  fashion  into  the  corresponding 
half  of  the  paired  vesicle  of  the  cerebrum  now  developed  into  the 
preponderant  cerebral  hemispheres,  the  two  cavities  of  which  are 


CHAP.  IL]  THE   BRAIN.  931 

now  known  as  the  lateral  ventricles.  The  growth  of  the  cerebral 
hemispheres  is  not  only  much  greater  than  that  of  the  rest  of  the 
brain,  but  also  takes  place  in  a  special  manner.  At  their  first 
appearance  the  cerebral  hemispheres  lie  wholly  in  front  of  the 
fore-brain  or  vesicle  of  the  third  ventricle,  but  in  their  subsequent 
growth  while  expanding  in  nearly  all  directions  they  extend 
especially  backwards.  Thus  in  the  adult  brain,  on  the  dorsal 
surface  they  not  only  completely  cover  up  the  third  ventricle  but 
also  overlap  the  mid-brain,  reaching  so  far  back  as  to  cover  the 
front  border  of  the  cerebellum,  while  on  the  ventral  surface,  though 
in  the  middle  line  they  leave  exposed  the  floor  or  ventral  'portions 
of  the  walls  of  the  third  ventricle,  at  the  sides  they  are  seen  to 
reach  as  far  backward  as  on  the  dorsal  surface.  The  median  furrow 
on  the  dorsal  surface  which  separates  each  hemisphere  from  its 
fellow  is  at  first  shallow  but  rapidly  deepens,  so  that  as  the  hemi- 
spheres grow  they  become  separated  from  each  other  by  a  narrow 
deep  longitudinal  fissure,  into  which  as  we  shall  see  a  fold  of  the 
dura  mater  dips.  This  fissure  is  not  only  deep  vertically,  i.e.  from 
the  dorsal  surface  ventrally,  but  at  the  front  of  the  brain  runs 
backward  in  the  middle  line  almost  as  far  as  the  level  of  the  third 
ventricle,  so  as  completely  to  separate  from  each  other  the  anterior 
parts  of  each  hemisphere,  known  as  the  anterior  lobes;  at  the 
back  of  the  brain  also  it  similarly  runs  forward  in  the  middle  line 
for  a  considerable  distance,  so  as  to  separate  from  each  other  the 
posterior  lobes.  Hence  the  two  great  masses  of  the  cerebral 
hemisphere  are  united  with  each  other,  not  along  their  whole 
length  but  for  about  a  third  of  that  length,  the  isthmus  or  bridge 
thus  connecting  them  lying  at  some  depth  below  the  dorsal 
surface  at  the  bottom  of  the  longitudinal  fissure,  in  about  the 
middle  third  of  its  length. 

At  its  first  appearance  each  lateral  ventricle  is  of  a  more  or  less 
oval  form,  its  walls  are  of  uniform  thickness,  and  it  lies  in  front  of 
the  third  ventricle.  During  the  growth  of  the  hemispheres  it 
acquires  a  peculiar  shape  and  becomes  divided  into  an  anterior 
cornu  or  horn  stretching  into  the  anterior  portion,  a  posterior 
horn  stretching  into  the  posterior  portion,  and  a  descending  horn, 
which  curves  laterally  and  ventrally  into  the  middle  portion  of  the 
hemisphere ;  owing  to  the  great  backward  extension  of  the  hemi- 

rsres  the  lateral  ventricles  come  to  lie  not  only  in  front  of  but 
at  the  side  of,  and  indeed,  to  a  certain  extent,  above  or  dorsal 
to  the  third  ventricle ;  and  during  the  growth  of  the  parts  the 
originally  wide  Y-shaped  opening  which  placed  the  hind  ends  of 
the  two  lateral  ventricles  in  communication  with  the  front  of  the 
third  ventricle  becomes  narrowed  into  a  slit-like  passage  of  similar 
form,  the  foramen  of  Monro,  which  still  opening  into  the  front  of 
the  third  ventricle,  now  leads  on  each  side  from  a  point  rather  in 
front  of  the  middle  of  the  lateral  ventricle. 

As  the  hemisphere  enlarges  the  growth  of  the  walls  of  the 


932  GENERAL   STRUCTURE.  [BOOK  in. 

vesicle  is  not  uniform  in  all  parts.  At  an  early  period  there  may 
be  observed  in  the  ventral  wall  or  floor  of  the  vesicle  a  thickening, 
which  assuming  a  special,  more  or  less  semilunar,  form  arid  pro- 
jecting into  the  cavity  becomes  the  body  known  as  the  corpus 
striatum.  As  development  proceeds  the  corpus  striatum  on  each 
side  becomes  attached  to  the  optic  thalamus,  lying  behind  and  to 
the  median  side  of  itself,  the  radiating  fibres  of  the  crus  cerebri 
passing  between  the  two,  and  also  as  we  shall  see  dividing* the 
corpus  striatum  into  two  bodies,  called  the  nucleus  caudatus  and 
nucleus  lenticularis.  A  notable  result  of  this  growth  and  change 
of  position  of  the  hemispheres  and  of  the  coalescence  of  the 
corpus  striatum  with  the  optic  thalamus  is  that  the  latter  body, 
though  really  belonging  to  the  third  ventricle,  comes  to  project 
somewhat  into  the  lateral  ventricle ;  a  strip  of  the  upper  surface 
of  the  optic  thalamus,  along  its  outer,  lateral  edge,  forms  a 
portion  of  the  floor  of  the  lateral  ventricle  in  the  median  region 
on  each  side  of  the  third  ventricle.  Besides  this  special  de- 
velopment of  the  corpus  striatum,  the  walls  of  each  vesicle,  with 
the  exception  of  the  median  part  by  which  the  two  vesicles  coalesce 
with  each  other,  become  (we  are  now  speaking  of  the  higher 
mammals)  thickened  much  in  the  same  way  all  over,  the  surface 
being  folded  so  as  to  give  rise  to  convolutions  or  gyri  separated 
by  furrows  or  sulci ;  and  the  thickening  taking  place  in  such  a 
way  as  to  give  the  ventricle  its  peculiar  shape.  The  median 
coalesced  part  undergoes  a  different  and  peculiar  change.  This 
part,  which  at  first  lies  in  front  of  the  third  ventricle,  through 
the  changes  brought  about  by  the  growth  of  the  hemispheres  so 
shifts  its  position  as  to  lie  immediately  over,  dorsal  to  the  third 
ventricle,  ,very  much  as  if  this  part  of  the  cerebral  vesicles  had 
been  folded  back  over  the  fore-brain.  In  the  junction  itself  we 
may  distinguish  a  dorsal  and  a  ventral  portion.  The  dorsal  portion 
is  developed  into  a  system  of  transverse  commissural  fibres  passing 
across  from  one  hemisphere  to  the  other.  In  the  median  region 
these  fibres  form  a  thick  compact  band,  called  the  corpus  callosum, 
which  may  be  exposed  to  view  at  the  bottom  of  the  longitudinal 
fissure,  while  on  each  side  they  spread  away  in  all  directions  to 
nearly  all  parts  of  the  surface  of  the  hemispheres,  passing  over  and 
helping  to  form  the  roof  of  the  lateral  ventricles.  The  band  is  not 
flat  but  curved  ventral  wards;  hence  in  a  longitudinal  vertical  section 
of  the  brain  taken  in  the  middle  line  it  presents  a  curved  form  with 
the  concavity  directed  ventralwards.  While  this  dorsal  portion  of 
the  junction  is  developed  at  the  sides  as  well  as  in  the  middle  line, 
the  ventral  portion  is  developed  in  the  median  region  only,  and 
that  in  a  special  way,  so  that  it  forms  below,  ventral  to,  the  corpus 
callosum  an  arched  plate,  in  the  shape  of  a  triangle  with  the  apex 
directed  forwards,  called  the  fornix,  which  lies  immediately  above 
the  thin  epithelial  roof  of  the  third  ventricle.  In  front,  the 
narrower  apical  portion  of  the  fornix  lies  at  some  little  distance 


CHAP,  ii.]  THE   BRAIN.  933 

below,  ventral  to,  the  corpus  callosum,  and  here  the  junction 
between  the  two  vesicles  is  reduced  to  a  thin  sheet,  the  septum 
lucidum;  but  behind,  the  broader  basal  portion  of  the  fornix  is 
arched  up  so  as  to  lie  immediately  under  and  touch  the  corpus 
callosum.  Hence  the  septum  lucidum  has  the  form  of  a  more  or 
less  triangular  vertical  sheet,  broad  in  front  and  narrowing  behind, 
separating  the  two  lateral  ventricles.  The  sheet  may  be  conceived 
of  as  being  double  and  formed  by  the  apposition  of  two  layers,  one 
belonging  to  each  ventricle ;  between  these  two  layers  is  developed 
a  narrow  closed  cavity  containing  fluid,  called  the  fifth  ventricle.  But 
while  the  lateral  ventricles  open  by  the  foramen  of  Monro  into  the 
third  ventricle  and  the  third  ventricle  is  continuous  by  means  of 
the  aqueduct  with  the  fourth  ventricle,  which  again  passes  into  the 
central  canal  of  the  spinal  cord,  the  whole  series  being  developed 
out  of  the  same  embryonic  neural  canal,  the  fifth  ventricle  com- 
municates with  none  of  them ;  it  is  a  cavity  of  different  origin. 

The  corpus  callosum  or  dorsal  portion  of  the  junction  between 
the  vesicles  spreads  out,  as  we  have  said,  laterally  along  its  whole 
length,  and  thus  forms  a  broad  band  joining  the  two  hemispheres 
together ;  the  middle  portion  spreads  out  in  a  more  or  less  straight 
direction  though  curving  over  the  ventricle  upwards  and  downwards 
to  reach  various  parts  of  the  hemisphere,  while  the  front  and  hind 
ends  bend  round  on  each  side  forwards  and  backwards  to  reach  the 
anterior  and  posterior  parts.  Thus  through  the  corpus  callosum 
the  thick  wall  of  one  ventricle  is  made  continuous  with  that  of 
the  other.  The  disposition  of  the  fornix  or  ventral  portion  of  the 
junction  is  very  different.  At  its  apex  in  front  the  fornix  bifur- 
cates into  two  bands,  known  as  the  pillars  of  the  fornix,  which  on 
each  side  become  continuous  with,  and  take  a  peculiar  course  in 
the  walls  of  the  third  ventricle.  In  like  manner  behind,  the  angles 
of  the  base  of  the  fornix  are  continuous  with  the  walls  of  the  lateral 
ventricles,  that  is  to  say,  with  the  thick  mass  of  the  hemispheres, 
being  also  prolonged  as  two  special  strands  of  fibres  called  the 
crura  of  the  fornix.  But  along  each  side  of  the  triangle,  between 
the  attachments  in  front  and  behind,  the  substance  of  the  fornix  is 
not  continued  into  the  substance  of  the  corresponding  hemisphere ; 
the  edge  of  the  fornix  appears  on  each  side  to  lie  loose  on  the 
dorsal  surface  of  the  optic  thalamus,  which  here  forms  the  median 
portion  of  the  floor  of  the  lateral  ventricle ;  between  the  optic 
thalamus  below  and  the  fornix  above  there  seems  to  be  a  narrow 
slit  by  which  the  cavity  of  the  lateral  ventricle  communicates  with 
parts  outside  itself.  In  reality  however  there  is  no  actual  breach 
of  continuity  though  there  is  a  breach  of  nervous  substance.  The 
slit  is  bridged  over  by  a  layer  of  epithelium,  by  means  of  which 
the  edge  of  the  fornix  is  made  continuous  with  the  upper  surface 
of  the  optic  thalamus,  and  the  median  wall  of  the  lateral  ventricle 
made  complete.  But  this  layer  of  epithelium  has  the  following 
peculiar  relations  to  the  pia  mater  covering  the  brain. 


934  GENERAL   STRUCTURE.  [BOOK  in. 

We  have  said  that  the  roof  of  the  third  ventricle,  like  that  of 
the  fourth  ventricle,  consists  only  of  a  layer  of  epithelium  devoid  of 
nervous  elements.  We  have  further  seen  that  the  fornix,  and  the 
hind  part  of  the  corpus  callosum  with  which  it  is  continuous 
overlie  the  third  ventricle,  the  free  base  of  the  fornix  with  the 
rounded  hind  end  of  the  corpus  callosum  above  forming  together 
the  hind  border  of  the  junction  or  bridge  between  the  two 
hemispheres.  The  pia  mater  covering  the  dorsal  surface  of  the 
brain,  passing  forwards  under  this  curved  border,  spreads  over  the 
top  of  the  third  ventricle,  becoming  adherent  to  the  layer  of 
epithelium  just  referred  to,  and  thus  forms  a  vascular  sheet  called 
the  velum  interpositum,  which  serves  as  the  actual  roof  of  the 
third  ventricle,  immediately  below,  ventral  to,  the  fornix ;  it 
cannot  be  seen  without  previously  removing  the  fornix.  At  the 
lateral  edge  of  the  fornix,  on  each  side,  this  same  vascular  sheet  of 
pia  mater  projects  from  beneath  the  fornix  into  the  lateral  ventricle 
carrying  with  it  the  layer  of  epithelium  which,  as  we  said,  made 
the  edge  of  the  fornix  actually  continuous  with  the  rest  of  the 
walls  of  the  lateral  ventricle ;  the  part  of  the  pia  mater  thus  seen 
projecting  beyond  the  edge  of  the  fornix  when  the  lateral  ventricle 
is  laid  open  is  called  the  choroid  plexus.  To  this  peculiar  intrusion 
of  the  pia  mater,  by  which  the  nutrition  of  the  brain  is  assisted, 
we  shall  return  when  we  come  to  speak  of  the  vascular  arrange- 
ments of  the  brain.  Meanwhile  we  may  point  out,  that  while  this 
vascular  ingrowth  seems  to  make  the  cavity  of  the  third  ventricle 
continuous  with  that  of  the  lateral  ventricle  on  each  side,  and  all 
three  with  the  exterior  of  the  brain,  it  really  does  not  do  so.  The 
cavity  of  the  third  ventricle  is  made  complete  by  the  layer  of 
epithelium  forming  its  roof,  and  the  cavity  of  the  lateral  ventricle 
is  made  complete  by  the  layer  of  epithelium  passing  from  the 
lateral  edge  of  the  fornix  over  the  choroid  plexus  to  the  other 
parts  of  the  wall  of  the  ventricle.  To  pass  along  this  line  from 
the  actual  cavity  of  the  lateral  into  that  of  the  third  ventricle  one 
must  first  pierce  the  epithelium  covering  the  choroid  plexus,  thus 
gaining  access  to  the  pia  mater  of  the  plexus  and  of  the  velum, 
and  then  again  pierce  the  epithelium  coating  the  under  surface 
of  the  velum  and  forming  the  roof  of  the  third  ventricle.  It  is 
only  by  the  foramen  of  Monro  that  a  real  communication  exists 
between  the  cavity  of  the  lateral  and  that  of  the  third  ventricle. 

Thus  by  the  large  growth  and  backward  extension  of  the 
cerebral  hemispheres,  the  third  ventricle  comes  to  form  as  it  were 
the  front  end  of  the  cerebrospinal  axis,  the  crura  cerebri  expanding 
on  each  side  of  the  third  ventricle  into  the  cerebral  hemispheres 
which  cover  up  the  ventricle  on  the  dorsal  surface  but  leave  its 
walls  exposed  on  the  ventral  surface.  Attached  to  the  dorsal 
surface  of  the  third  ventricle  at  its  hind  end,  ventral  to  and 
somewhat  projecting  beyond  the  base  of  the  fornix,  lies  the  pineal 
gland  with  its  attachments,  the^  remnants  of  a  once-important 


CHAP.  IL]  THE   BRAIN.  935 

median  organ ;  and  attached  to  the  ventral  surface  of  the 
ventricle,  at  the  apex  of  a  funnel-shaped  projection,  the  infun- 
dibulum,  lies  the  pituitary  body,  also  a  remnant  of  important 
ancestral  structures. 

§  603.  We  may  then  divide  the  whole  brain  into  a  series  of 
parts  corresponding  to  the  main  divisions  of  the  embryonic  brain. 
At  the  front  lie  the  cerebral  hemispheres,  with  the  lateral  ventricles, 
developed  out  of  the  cerebral  vesicles;  and  with  these  are  asso- 
ciated the  corpora  striata,  the  term  cerebral  hemisphere  being 
sometimes  used  so  as  to  include  these  bodies,  and  sometimes  so 
as  to  exclude  them.  Next  come,  corresponding  to  the  original 
fore-brain,  the  parts  forming  the  walls  of  the  third  ventricle, 
conspicuous  among  which  are  the  optic  thalami ;  for  these 
bodies  though  they  appear  to  intrude  into  the  lateral  ventricles 
belong  properly  to  the  third  ventricle.  In  the  mid-brain  which 
follows,  the  cavity,  now  the  tubular  passage  of  the  aqueduct,  is 
roofed  in  by  the  two  pairs,  anterior  and  posterior,  of  corpora  quad- 
rigemina,  the  dimensions  of  which  are  not  very  great;  but  a 
thick  floor  is  furnished  by  the  crura  cerebri.  In  each  crus  we 
must  distinguish  between  a  dorsal  portion  called  the  tegmentum, 
in  which  a  large  quantity  of  grey  matter  is  present  and  in  which  a 
great  complexity  in  the  arrangement  of  fibres  exists,  and  a  ventral 
portion,  the  pes  or  crusta,  which  is  a  much  more  uniform  mass  of 
longitudinally  disposed  fibres.  As  the  crura  passing  forward  diverge 
into  the  cerebral  hemisphere  on  each  side,  the  tegmentum  ceases 
at  the  hinder  end  and  ventral  parts  of  the  optic  thalamus;  it 
is  the  pes  which  supplies  the  mass  of  fibres  radiating  into  each 
cerebral  hemisphere.  In  a  view  of  the  ventral  surface  of  the 
brain,  the  base  of  the  brain  as  it  is  frequently  called,  the  crura 
may  be  seen  emerging  from  the  anterior  border  of  the  pons.  This 
we  have  spoken  of  as  the  thickened  floor  of  the  front  part  of  the 
hind  brain,  but  in  reality,  it  encroaches  a  little  on  the  mid-brain, 
the  hind  part  of  the  corpora  quadrigemina  being  in  the  same 
dorso ventral  plane  as  the  front  part  of  the  pons  (see  Fig.  108). 
In  the  main,  however,  the  pons  belongs  to  the  fore  part  of  the 
hind-brain,  the  roof  and  sides  of  which  are  developed  as  we  have 
said  into  the  cerebellum.  This  superficially  resembles  the  cerebral 
hemispheres  in  its  large  size,  and  in  the  special  development  of  its 
surface,  which  is  formed  of  grey  matter  folded  in  a  remarkable 
manner  and  often  spoken  of  as  cortex.  The  cerebellum,  though 
the  lateral  portions,  called  the  hemispheres,  project  above  the 
median  portion,  called  the  vermis,  is,  unlike  the  cerebrum,  a 
single  mass ;  each  lateral  half  however  sends  down  ventrally  a 
mass  of  fibres  which,  running  transversely,  partly  end  in  the 
pons  and  partly  are  continued  across  the  pons  into  the  other 
lateral  half;  this  mass  of  fibres,  thus  constituting  as  we  have 
said  a  considerable  part  of  the  pons,  forms  on  each  side,  just  as 
it  leaves  the  cerebellum  to  enter  the  pons,  a  thick  strand,  called 


936  GENERAL   STRUCTURE.  [BOOK  in. 

the  middle  peduncle  of  the  cerebellum.  From  the  cerebellum 
there  also  proceeds  backwards  into  the  bulb  on  each  side  a  thick 
strand  of  fibres,  the  inferior  peduncle  of  the  cerebellum  or  restiform 
body ;  and  a  third  strand,  the  superior  peduncle  of  the  cerebellum, 
passes  forwards  on  each  side  into  the  region  of  the  corpora 
quadrigemina.  As  the  latter  converge  towards  each  other  behind 
the  corpora  quadrigemina  the  angle  between  them  is  filled  up  by 
a  thin  sheet  of  nervous  matter,  the  valve  of  Vieussens,  which  thus 
for  a  little  distance  backwards  forms  a  roof  for  the  front  part  of  the 
fourth  ventricle,  just  where  the  lozenge-shaped  cavity  is  narrowing 
to  become  the  aqueduct.  Behind  the  cerebellum  and  pons  comes 
the  bulb,  which  as  we  have  said  is  the  thickened  floor  of  the  hind 
part  of  the  hind-brain,  the  roof  of  the  cavity  being  here  practically 
wanting. 

Of  these  several  divisions,  the  first  division,  that  of  the  cerebral 
hemispheres,  including  the  corpora  striata,  stands  apart  from  the 
rest  by  reason  both  of  its  origin  and  the  character  of  its  develop- 
ment. As  we  shall  see,  this  anatomical  distinction  corresponds  to 
a  physiological  difference. 

Of  the  other  parts  of  the  brain  the  crura  cerebri  deserve 
special  attention.  We  may  regard  these  as  starting  in  the  cord 
but  largely  augmented  in  the  bulb ;  they  traverse  the  pons,  where 
they  are  still  further  increased,  and  passing  beneath  the  corpora 
quadrigemina,  with  which  as  well  as  with  the  cerebellum  they 
make  connections,  end  partly  in  the  region  of  the  optic  thalami 
and  walls  of  the  third  ventricle,  but  to  a  great  extent  in  the 
cerebral  hemispheres.  We  may  in  a  certain  sense  consider  the 
rest  of  the  brain  as  built  upon  and  attached  to  these  fundamental 
basal  or  ventral  strands. 

§  604.  Connected  with  the  brain  are  a  series  of  paired  nerves, 
the  cranial  nerves.  The  first  and  second  pair,  the  olfactory  nerves 
and  the  optic  nerves,  differ  in  their  origin  and  mode  of  develop- 
ment from  all  the  rest  so  fundamentally  as  to  cause  regret  that 
they  are  included  in  the  same  category.  We  shall  consider  these 
by  themselves  in  due  course.  The  remaining  pairs,  from  the  third 
pair  to  the  twelfth,  forming  a  much  more  homogeneous  category,  we 
shall  also  consider  in  their  proper  place.  We  must  now  turn  to 
study  in  greater  detail  some  of  the  structural  features  of  the 
brain,  and  we  may  with  advantage  begin  with  the  bulb. 


SEC.   2.     THE   BULB. 


§  605.  The  spinal  cord,  as  it  ascends  to  the  brain,  becomes 
changed  into  the  more  complex  bulb,  partly  by  a  shifting  of  the 
course  of  the  tracts  of  white  fibres,  partly  by  an  opening  up  of 
the  narrow  central  canal  into  the  wide  and  superficial  fourth 
ventricle,  but  chiefly  by  the  development  of  new  grey  matter. 

When  the  anterior,  ventral,  aspect  of  the  bulb  is  examined 
(Fig.  108,  C),  it  will  be  seen  that  the  anterior  columns  of  the  cord 
are  interrupted  for  some  distance  in  the  median  line  by  bundles 
of  fibres  (Py.  dec)  which,  appearing  to  rise  up  from  deeper  parts, 
cross  over  from  side  to  side  and  so  confuse  the  line  of  the  anterior 
fissure.  This  is  the  decussation  of  the  pyramids,  above  which  the 
place  of  the  anterior  columns  of  the  spinal  cord  is  taken  by  two 
larger,  more  prominent  columns,  the  pyramids  of  the  bulb  (Py.\ 
which  are  continued  forwards  to  the  hind  margin  of  the  pons. 
On  the  outer  side  of,  lateral  to,  each  pyramid,  lies  a  projecting  oval 
mass,  the  olivary  body  or  inferior  olive  (ol.)  separating  the  pyramid 
from  a  column  of  white  matter,  the  restiform  body  (R),  which, 
occupying  the  lateral  region  of  the  bulb,  when  traced  backwards 
appears  to  continue  the  line  of  the  lateral  column  of  the  cord,  and 
when  traced  forwards  is  seen  to  run  up  to  the  cerebellum  as  the 
inferior  peduncle  of  that  organ.  On  the  posterior  dorsal  aspect 
no  such  decussation  is  seen.  The  two  posterior  columns  of  the 
cord  diverge  from  each  other,  leaving  between  them  a  triangular 
space,  the  calamus  scriptorius,  which  is  the  hind  part  of  the 
lozenge-shaped  shallow  cavity  of  the  fourth  ventricle.  As  the 
cord  passes  into  the  bulb  the  posterior  column  as  a  whole  grows 
broader,  and  the  division  into  a  median  posterior  and  an  external 
posterior  column  becomes  very  obvious  and  distinct  by  the 
appearance  of  a  conspicuous  furrow  separating  the  two.  At 
some  distance  however  in  front  of  the  point  of  divergence  of 
the  columns  or  apex  of  the  calamus  scriptorius,  the  furrow 
becomes  less  marked,  and  it  eventually  fades  away.  In  its  course 
the  furrow  takes  such  a  line  that  the  median  posterior  column, 
forming  the  immediate  lateral  boundary  of  the  fourth  ventricle, 


SURFACE  VIEWS. 


[BOOK  m. 


«f  S?;.S 


<        :    M'sI-riS'S* 

itlillff* 


Mj8|-i§^ 

O 


S    I§l^l-3§S 


CHAP,  ii.]  THE   BRAIN.  939 

has  the  appearance  of  a  strand  broad  behind  but  thinning  away 
in  front,  while  the  external  posterior  column,  also  broadening  as  it 
advances  forwards,  seems  to  be  wedged  in  between  the  median 
posterior  column  on  its  median  edge  and  the  restiform  body  on  its 
lateral  edge ;  hence  the  former  is  here  called  the  fasciculus  (or 
funiculus)  gracilis  (m.  p.),  and  the  latter  the  fasciculus  (or  funi- 
culus) cuneatus  (e.  p.).  Further  forward  both  columns  seem  to 
merge  with  each  other  and  with  fibres  which  curve  round  to  form 
part  of  the  restiform  body;  the  relations  however  of  these  two 
columns  to  each  other  and  to  the  other  parts  of  the  bulb,  as  well 
as  the  nature  of  the  other  several  changes  by  which  the  cord  is 
transformed  into  the  bulb,  are  disclosed  by  transverse  vertical 
(dorso-ventral)  sections,  to  the  study  of  which  we  must  now  turn. 

A  section  (Fig.  109,  1)  taking  at  the  hind  margin  of  the 
decussation,  at  which  level  the  first  cervical  nerve  takes  origin, 
when  compared  with  a  section  of  the  cord  at  the  level  of  the 
second  cervical  nerve  (cf.  Fig.  104,  C2),  shews  that  certain  changes 
are  already  taking  place  in  the  grey  matter.  The  anterior  horns 
are  not  much  altered,  but  the  posterior  horns  are,  as  it  were, 
pushed  out  laterally  and  dorsally  so  that  the  posterior  columns, 
which  as  yet  retain  their  previous  great  depth,  become  very 
much  broader  than  they  are  lower  down,  encroaching,  so  to 
speak,  on  the  lateral  columns.  At  the  same  time  the  substance 
of  Rolando  (s.  </.),  forming  the  head  or  caput  of  the  horn,  has 
enlarged  into  a  more  or  less  globular  form,  and  lies  near  the 
surface  of  the  cord  though  separated  from  it  by  a  compact  tract 
of  longitudinal  fibres  (V.  a.),  which  as  we  shall  see,  belongs  to  the 
fifth  cranial  nerve.  A  considerable  development  of  the  reticular 
formation  (/.  ret.)  at  the  side  of  the  grey  matter  ventral  to  the 
posterior  horn  has  also  taken  place,  and  this  with  the  shifting 
of  the  position  of  the  posterior  horn  has  driven  the  lateral  horn 
(l.h.)  nearer  to  the  anterior  horn.  From  this  lateral  horn  a  root 
of  the  eleventh  spinal  accessory  cranial  nerve  (xi),  may  be  seen 
taking  origin.  Further,  a  great  increase  of  grey  matter  round  the 
central  canal  may  also  be  observed. 

These  changes,  however,  are  of  degree  only ;  what  seems  to 
be  an  absolutely  new  feature  is  the  presence  of  bundles  of  fibres 
(Py.  dec.),  which  starting  from  the  anterior  column  of  one  side 
cross  over  to  and  are  apparently  lost  in  the  grey  matter  of  the 
neck  of  the  anterior  horn  of  the  other  side ;  in  so  crossing  the 
fibres  push  aside  the  bottom  of  the  anterior  fissure.  When  the 
course  of  these  fibres  is  investigated,  either  by  simple  microscopic 
observation,  or  still  better  by  the  method  of  degeneration,  it  is 
found  that  they  may  be  traced  from  the  anterior  column  of  one 
side,  across  the  anterior  commissure,  through  the  neck  of  the 
anterior  horn  to  the  lateral  column  of  the  opposite  side,  and  to 
that  part  of  the  lateral  column  which  we  have  previously  de- 
scribed as  the  crossed  pyramidal  tract. 


940 


DECUSSATION   OF   PYRAMIDS.  [BOOK  in. 


m.p.n 


ar.C.i 


m/P-n-e.p.n. 


XII 


-fret. 


Py.dec. 


bl.e. 


ol.a 


fa.i!  (n.a.. 


'fa.e. 


FIG.  109.     TRANSVERSE  DORSOVENTRAL  SECTIONS  OF  THE  BULB  (MAN)  AT 
DIFFERENT  LEVELS.     (Sherrington). 


CHAP,  ii.]  THE   BRAIN.  941 

This  and  Figs.  110 — 114,  form  a  series  of  transverse  dorsoventral  sections  of 
the  brain  taken  at  different  levels  from  the  hind  end  of  the  bulb  to  the  front  of  the 
third  ventricle  ;  the  several  levels  are  shewn  by  the  lines  drawn  in  Fig.  108.  They 
are  all  magnified  twice.  The  details  are  shewn,  for  the  sake  of  simplicity,  in 
diagrammatic  fashion ;  the  white  matter  is  left  unshaded,  the  course  of  the  fibres 
being  indicated  in  a  few  important  instances  only  ;  the  grey  matter  is  shaded 
formally,  the  nerve-cells  being  indicated  in  the  case  only  of  the  nuclei  of  the  cranial 
nerves.  The  want  of  complete  bilateral  symmetry  which  is  often  met  with  in  such 
sections  is  indicated  in  several  of  the  figures. 

1.  At  the  hind  limit  of  the  decussation  of  the  pyramids.  2.  In  the  middle  of 
the  decussation.  3.  At  the  upper  end  of  the  decussation.  4.  Just  below 
the  point  of  the  calamus  scriptorius.  5.  Just  above  the  point.  6.  Through 
the  middle  of  the  ala  cinerea. 

Py.  Pyramids.  Py.  dec.  decussation  of  the  pyramids.  Supra  Py.  dec.  superior 
decussation.  /.  a.  i.  internal  arcuate  fibres.  /.  a.  e.  external  arcuate  fibres. 
Cb.  position  of  cerebellar  tract.  R.  restiform  body  or  inferior  peduncle  of  the 
cerebellum,  e.  p.  external  posterior  column,  fasciculus  cuneatus,  m.  p.  median 
posterior  column,  fasciculus  gracilis.  r.  raphe. 

I.  h.  lateral  horn.  m.  p.  n.  nucleus  of  the  median  posterior  column  or  gracile 
nucleus,  e.  p.  n.  nucleus  of  the  external  posterior  column  or  cuneate  nucleus. 
e.  p.  n.  (m.)  median  division  and  e.  p.  n.  (L)  lateral  division  of  the  same. 
ol.  olivary  body.  ol.  a.  median  accessory,  and  ol.  e.  lateral  accessory  olive. 
in.  ol.  interolivary  layer,  a.  1.  n.  lateral  (antero -lateral)  nucleus,  n.  a.  arcuate 
nucleus,  a.  c.  remnant  of  anterior  horn.  /.  ret.  reticular  formation,  s.  g. 
substance  of  Kolando. 

a.  r.  c.  I.  anterior  root,  and#.  r.  c.  I.  posterior  root  of  first  cervical  nerve.  XI.  root 
of  spinal  accessory  nerve.  XII.  twelfth  or  hypoglossal  nerve,  n.  XII.  nucleus 
of  the  same  in  6 ;  the  nucleus  may  be  traced  however  through  2,  3,  4,  5,  in 
connection  with  the  fibres  of  the  nerve,  s.  X.  sensory  or  main  part  of  the 
glossopharyngeal-vago-accessory  nucleus.  X.  m.  motor  nucleus  of  the  vagus, 
or  nucleus  ambiguus.  IX.  a.  ascending  root  of  the  glossopharyngeal  nucleus. 
V.  a.  ascending  root  of  the  fifth  nerve. 

4th.  fourth  ventricle ;  the  ependyma  or  lining  is  indicated  by  a  thick  dark  line ; 
and  in  5  and  6,  the  tooth-like  section  of  the  projecting  obex  is  shewn. 

In  a  section  a  little  higher  up  (Fig.  109,  2),  these  decussating 
fibres  form  on  each  side  a  large  strand  which  starts  from  a  part 
of  the  anterior  column,  now  becoming  distinctly  marked  off  as 
the  pyramid  (Py.),  and  is  apparently  lost  in  the  reticular  forma- 
tion, but  in  reality  passes  on  to  the  crossed  pyramidal  tract  of 
the  lateral  column.  This  strand,  as  it  crosses  over,  completely 
cuts  off  the  head  of  the  anterior  horn  from  the  more  central 
grey  matter,  and  forms  with  its  fellow  a  large  area  of  decussating 
fibres  between  the  bottom  of  the  anterior  fissure  and  the  central 
grey  matter.  When  a  surface  view  of  the  bulb  is  examined  the 
decussation  is  seen  to  be  effected  by  alternate  bundles,  passing 
now  from  right  to  left,  now  from  left  to  right ;  and  in  transverse 
sections  we  find  correspondingly  that  the  anterior  fissure  appears 
bent  now  to  the  left  and  now  to  the  right,  according  as  the 
section  cuts  through  a  bundle  passing  from  left  to  right  or  from 
right  to  left. 

In  sections  still  higher  up  (Fig.  109,  3  and  4)  this  conspicuous 
strand  of  fibres  crossing  obliquely  from  side  to  side,  will  be  no 
longer  seen ;  decussating  fibres  are  seen  dorsal  to  the  anterior 
fissure,  but  these,  of  which  we  shall  speak  presently,  are  of 


942  DECUSSATION   OF   PYRAMIDS.  [Boon  in. 

different  nature  and  origin.  The  fibres  which  in  sections  below 
were  seen  in  the  act  of  crossing  are  now  gathered  into  masses 
of  longitudinal  fibres,  the  pyramids,  (Py.)  one  on  each  side  of  the 
anterior  fissure,  each  with  a  sectional  area  of  a  rounded  triangular 
form  clearly  marked  out  from  the  surrounding  structures;  the 
section  is  taken  above  the  decussation  of  the  pyramids.  Or, 
tracing  the  changes  from  below  upwards  we  may  say  that  the 
decussation  is  now  complete ;  on  each  side  the  whole  of  the 
crossed  pyramidal  tract  of  the  spinal  cord  has,  in  the  region  of 
the  bulb  below  the  level  of  the  present  sections,  crossed  over  to 
the  other  side,  and  joining  with  the  direct  pyramidal  tract  of 
the  anterior  column  of  the  cord  of  the  same  side  has  become  the 
pyramid  of  the  bulb.  In  other  words,  the  decussation  of  the 
pyramids  is,  as  we  have  already  hinted,  the  passing  off  from 
each  pyramid,  and  the  crossing  over  to  the  opposite  side  of  the 
cord,  of  those  fibres  which  are  destined  to  become  the  crossed 
pyramidal  tract  of  the  spinal  cord  of  the  opposite  side,  while 
the  rest  of  the  pyramid  pursues  its  course  on  the  same  side  as 
the  direct  pyramidal  tract. 

§  606.  In  the  spinal  cord  the  bottom  of  the  anterior  fissure  is 
separated  from  the  central  canal  by  nothing  more  than  the 
anterior  white  commissure  and  a  narrow  band  of  grey  matter, 
composed  of  the  anterior  grey  commissure  and  of  part  of  the 
central  gelatinous  substance.  During  the  decussation  of  the 
pyramids,  the  decussating  fibres  push,  as  it  were,  the  central 
canal  with  its  surrounding  grey  matter  to  some  distance  from 
the  bottom  of  the  anterior  fissure.  In  sections  above  the  decus- 
sation the  bottom  of  the  fissure  does  not  again  approach  the 
central  canal,  but  continues  to  be  removed  to  some  distance  from 
it,  and,  as  we  pass  upwards,  to  an  increasing  distance,  by  the 
interposition  of  tissue  which  consists  largely  of  decussating  fibres. 
These  however,  though  they  seem  to  continue  on  the  decussation 
of  the  pyramids,  are  shewn  by  the  embryological  and  degeneration 
methods  to  have  no*  connection  with  the  pyramids,  but  to  belong 
to  another  system  of  decussation.  As  we  have  seen  (§  565)  the 
anterior  commissure  along  the  whole  length  of  the  cord  contains 
decussating  fibres.  Some  of  these  in  the  upper  part  of  the  cord 
are  fibres  crossing  from  the  direct  pyramidal  tract  of  one  side  to 
the  grey  matter  of  the  other  side,  and  so  may  be  regarded  as  part 
of  the  whole  pyramidal  tract ;  but  others  are  of  different  origin ; 
and  even  in  the  region  of  the  actual  decussation  of  the  pyramids 
some  of  the  fibres  which  cross  over  do  not  belong  to  the  pyramidal 
tract.  This  system  of  decussating  fibres  becomes  increasingly  pro- 
minent above  the  decussation  of  the  pyramids,  and  through  it  the 
ventral  area  of  the  bulb  between  the  central  canal  and  the  anterior 
fissure  is  much  increased.  The  fibres  as  they  cross  form  a  middle 
line  of  partition,  the  raphe  (Fig.  109,  4,  5,  r),  which  increases  in 
depth  in  the  upper  parts  of  the  bulb,  and  on  each  side  of  the  raphe 


CHAP,  ii.]  THE   BRAIN.  943 

help  to  break  up  the  grey  matter  (which  previously  formed  the 
anterior  horns)  into  what  is  called  the  reticular  formation.  We 
shall  return  to  this  presently,  but  may  here  call  attention  to  a 
special  development  of  these  decussating  fibres  which  is  seen 
just  above  the  decussation  of  the  pyramids.  In  a  section  at  this 
level  (Fig.  109,  3)  a  strand  of  fibres  (supra  Py.  dec.)  may  be  seen 
to  start  chiefly  from  the  gracile  nucleus  but  also  to  some  extent 
from  the  cuneate  nucleus,  to  sweep  round  the  central  grey 
matter,  and  to  decussate  ventral  to  this  between  it  and  the 
bottom  of  the  anterior  fissure.  This  is  called  the  superior  de- 
cussation, or,  for  reasons  which  we  shall  see  later  on,  the  sensory 
decussation. 

§  607.  We  must  now  turn  to  the  posterior  fissure  and  its 
relations  to  the  fourth  ventricle.  We  saw  that  at  the  beginning 
of  the  pyramidal  decussation,  the  posterior  horns  had  been 
thrown  backwards  and  outwards  so  as  to  increase  the  posterior 
columns.  The  posterior  fissure  is  still  of  great  depth,  so  that  by 
the  increase  of  breadth  and  maintenance  of  depth  the  posterior 
column,  the  lateral  limit  of  which  is  still  sharply  marked  out 
by  the  swollen  head  of  the  posterior  horn  as  well  as  by  the  highest 
posterior  rootlets  of  the  first  cervical  nerve,  acquires  at  this  level 
its  maximum  of  bulk. 

From  this  point  forward  the  depth  of  the  posterior  fissure  and 
the  dorso-ventral  diameter  of  the  posterior  columns  diminishes. 
The  head  of  the  horn  (Fig.  109,  2)  is  thrown  still  further  outwards 
into  the  lateral  regions ;  developments  of  grey  matter  at  the  base 
and  to  some  extent  at  the  neck  of  the  horn  (of  these  we  shall 
speak  presently)  encroach  (Fig.  109,  3)  dorsally  on  the  white 
matter  of  the  columns ;  and  the  central  grey  matter  appears  to 
rise  dorsally  at  the  expense  of  the  posterior  fissure,  in  coincidence 
with  the  development  described  above  as  taking  place  on  the 
ventral  side  of  the  canal. 

Still  a  little  further  forward,  in  a  section  for  instance  (Fig.  109, 
4)  a  little  way  behind  the  apex  of  the  calamus  scriptorius,  the 
central  grey  matter,  which  still  forms  a  rounded  mass  around  the 
central  canal,  is  brought  yet  nearer  to  the  posterior  fissure. 

In  a  section  yet  a  little  further  forward  (Fig.  109,  5)  carried 
through  the  hinder  narrow  part  of  the  fourth  ventricle  itself,  it  is 
seen  that  the  central  canal  has  opened  out  on  to  the  dorsal  surface, 
and  that  the  grey  matter,  which  in  previous  sections  surrounded 
it,  is  now  exposed  to  the  surface  on  the  floor  of  the  ventricle,  the 
median  posterior  columns  being  thrust  aside.  In  a  still  more 
forward  section  (Fig.  109,  6)  this  grey  matter  in  correspondence 
with  the  increasing  width  of  the  ventricle  occupies  a  still  wider 
area,  thrusting  still  further  aside  the  narrowing  upper  ends  of  the 
two  posterior  columns. 

During  these  successive  changes,  the  large  wide  posterior 
(both  external  posterior  and  median  posterior)  columns  of  the 

F.  60 


944  THE   RETICULAR   FORMATION.  [BOOK  in. 

cervical  spinal  cord  and  beginning  bulb,  are  reduced  to  small 
dimensions  and  in  the  end  disappear  ;  but  before  we  speak  of  the 
course  and  fate  of  the  tracts  of  fibres  constituting  these  columns 
we  must  turn  to  the  important  changes  of  the  grey  matter. 

§  608.  A  transverse  section  through  the  lower  end  of  the 
decussation  (Fig.  109,  1)  shews,  as  we  have  said,  few  differences 
as  regards  the  grey  matter  from  one  taken  at  the  level  of  the 
second  cervical  nerve.  The  changes  noticeable  are  mainly  the 
changes  in  position  of  the  posterior  horns,  the  increase  of  central 
grey  matter  around  the  central  canal,  the  approach  of  the  lateral 
horn,  from  which  spring  the  roots  of  the  spinal  accessory  nerve, 
to  the  anterior  horn,  and  an  increase  of  the  reticular  formation 
in  the  bay  ventral  to  the  posterior  horn. 

In  the  middle  of  the  decussation  (Fig.  109,  2)  the  decussating 
fibres  are  cutting  the  head  of  the  anterior  horn  away  from  the 
base  of  the  horn  and  the  central  grey  substance,  and  the  isolated 
head  is  diminishing  in  size,  being  separated  from  the  surface  of 
the  cord  by  an  increasing  thickness  of  white  matter.  The  lateral 
horn  and  origin  of  the  spinal  accessory  root  do  not  share  in  this 
isolation,  but  are  driven  back  again  dorsally  towards  the  posterior 
root  to  join  the  reticular  formation  which  is  increasing  in  area, 
while  the  lateral  column  of  white  matter  is  diminishing  in  bulk 
by  the  withdrawal  of  the  pyramidal  tract. 

Still  a  little  further  forward,  the  anterior  horn  seems  at  first 
sight  to  have  wholly  disappeared  (Fig.  109,  3  and  4),  but  its 
disappearance  is  coincident  with  an  increase  of  the  reticular 
formation  in  the  position  of  the  lateral  columns,  as  well  as  with 
the  growth  of  tissue  mentioned  above  between  the  anterior  fissure 
and  the  central  grey  matter.  In  fact,  between  the  anterior  pyra- 
mids on  the  ventral  side  and  the  largely  increased  and  laterally 
expanded  grey  matter  on  the  dorsal  side,  a  large  area  of  peculiar 
tissue  now  extends  on  each  side  for  a  considerable  distance  from 
the  middle  line  of  the  raphe,  encroaching  on  what  was  the  lateral 
column  of  white  matter;  and  a  corresponding  area  of  similar 
tissue  may  be  traced  from  this  level  through  the  higher  parts  of 
the  bulb  up  into  the  pons  and  crura  cerebri.  The  tissue  consists 
of  nerve  fibres  running  transversely,  longitudinally,  and  in  other 
directions,  so  as  to  form  a  network,  the  bars  of  which  are  often 
curved ;  and  with  these  fibres  are  found  branched  nerve  cells  in 
considerable  number,  some  of  them  small,  both  fibres  and  cells 
being  as  elsewhere  embedded  in  neuroglia.  Though  differing 
from  the  ordinary  grey  matter  of  the  cord  by  the  more  open 
character  of  its  network,  it  may  be  considered  as  a  form  of  grey 
matter.  We  may  consider  it  as  being  in  reality  the  grey  matter 
of  the  apparently  lost  anterior  horn  broken  up  and  dispersed  by 
the  passage  of  a  large  number  of  fibres  and  bundles  of  fibres, 
especially  of  the  decussating  fibres  spoken  of  in  §  606,  which 
since  they  curve  through  this  area  from  the  middle  line  laterally 


CHAP.  IL]  THE   BRAIN.  945 

are  called  arcuate  or  arciform  fibres,  internal  arcuate  fibres  (Fig. 
109,  6,  /.  a.  i.)  to  distinguish  them  from  the  external  arcuate 
fibres  (f.  a.  e.)  of  which  we  shall  speak  presently.  Fragments 
of  more  compact  grey  matter,  also  belonging  probably  to  the 
anterior  horn,  are  seen  at  intervals  in  this  area,  Fig.  109,  6,  ac. 
arid  elsewhere.  We  have  seen  that  nearly  all  the  way  along 
the  cord  the  grey  matter  of  the  neck  of  the  posterior  horn  is 
similarly  broken  up  by  bundles  of  fibres  into  what  we  there  called 
the  reticular  formation  (Figs.  98,  99,  r.f.  p.  and  r.f.  1.)',  and  this 
area  in  the  bulb  though  it  possesses  characters  of  its  own  is  also 
called  the  reticular  formation.  In  the  more  lateral  portion  of 
this  formation,  the  network  is  more  open  and  irregular,  the  fibres 
are  finer,  and  the  nerve  cells  are  more  abundant  than  in  the 
median  portion  where  the  nerve  cells,  except  in  the  immediate 
neighbourhood  of  the  raphe,  are  less  numerous  or  even  absent, 
and  the  fibres  are  coarser.  These  two  parts  are  sometimes  distin- 
guished as  the  outer  or  lateral,  and  the  inner  or  median  formation. 
In  the  middle  line  the  fibres  distinctly  interlace  and  decussate 
in  an  oblique  manner,  some  running  nearly  vertically  in  the  dorso- 
ventral  plane,  thus  constituting  as  we  have  said  a  thick  raphe, 
which,  however,  at  its  edges  gradually  merges  into  the  more  open 
network. 

§  609.  Within  the  area,  bounded  by  the  pyramids  ventrally, 
the  expanded  grey  matter  dorsally,  the  raphe  in  the  middle  line, 
and  the  white  matter  laterally,  certain  distinct  compact  masses 
of  grey  matter  make  their  appearance,  as  we  pass  upward  towards 
the  pons. 

One  of  the  most  important  of  these  gives  rise  to  the  olivary 
body,  or  inferior  olive  which,  as  we  have  seen,  projects  as  an  oval 
mass  (Fig.  108,  ol.)  on  each  side  of  the  pyramids,  reaching  from 
a  level  which  is  somewhat  higher  up  than  the  lower  limit  of  the 
pyramids,  almost  but  not  quite  to  the  pons.  The  olivary  body, 
as  a  whole,  consists  partly  of  white  matter,,  that  is  of  fibres,  and 
partly  of  grey  matter,  sometimes  called  the  olivary  nucleus.  This 
latter  is  disposed  in  the  form  of  a  hollow  flask  or  curved  bowl,  with 
deeply  folded  or  plaited  walls,  having  a  wide  open  mouth  directed 
inwards  towards  the  middle  line,  and  forwards  towards  the  pons 
(Fig.  109,  4,  5,  6,  ol.).  The  flask  is  filled  within  by  white  matter, 
and  covered  up  on  its  outside  with  white  matter  as  well  as  traversed 
by  fibres.  The  grey  matter  thus  forming  this  flask-shaped 
nucleus  consists  of  small  rounded  nerve  cells,  lying  in  a  bed  of 
tissue  which  is  partly  ordinary  neuroglia,  and  partly  a  fine  nervous 
network. 

Lying  to  the  median  side  of  the  olivary  body,  immediately 
dorsal  to  the  anterior  pyramid  is  another  small  mass  of  grey 
matter,  in  the  form  of  a  disc,  appearing  in  transverse  sections  as 
a  thick  bent  rod,  in  some  sections  consisting  of  two  parts  (Fig.  109, 
4,  ol.  a).  This  is  the  accessory  olivary  nucleus.  A  very  similar 

60—2 


946  THE   GREY   MATTER   OF   THE   BULB.     [Boon  m. 

body  lies  dorsal  to  the  olivary  nucleus,  in  the  lateral  reticular 
formation ;  this  is  also  called  an  accessory  olivary  nucleus,  being 
distinguished  (Fig.  109,  6,  ol.  e)  by  the  name  outer  accessory  nucleus 
from  the  above  mentioned  inner  accessory  nucleus.  It  will  be 
observed  in  these  transverse  sections  that  the  inner  accessory 
nucleus  is  separated  from  the  olivary  nucleus  by  a  bundle  of 
white  fibres  (Fig.  109,  4,  5,  6,  xn)  which,  running  ventrally  from 
the  grey  matter  in  the  dorsal  region,  comes  to  the  surface  between 
the  anterior  pyramids  and  the  olivary  body.  This  is  the  hypo- 
glossal  or  twelfth  cranial  nerve. 

On  the  surface  of  the  anterior  pyramid  itself  is  seen  on  each 
side  a  small  mass  of  grey  matter  (Fig.  109,  5,  6,  n.  a.),  which  since 
it  appears  to  be  connected  with  a  system  of  superficial  transverse 
fibres,  which  we  shall  describe  directly  as  the  external  arcuate 
fibres  (Fig.  109,  3,  4,  5,  6,  /.  a.  e.),  is  called  the  arcuate  nucleus. 
It  seems  to  belong  to  the  same  group  as  the  accessory  olives. 

Lastly,  a  small  somewhat  diffuse  collection  of  grey  matter 
is  seen  in  sections  as  a  rounded  mass  of  irregular  form  placed 
lateral  to  the  reticular  formation  (Fig.  109,  4,  5,  6,  a.  I.  n).  This, 
which  at  its  first  appearance  seems  to  be  budded  off  from  the 
general  mass  of  grey  matter  (Fig.  109,  3,  a.  I.  n)  and  which  is 
probably  a  detached  portion  of  the  base  of  the  anterior  horn  or  of 
the  lateral  region  of  the  grey  matter,  is  called  the  lateral  or  antero- 
lateral  nucleus. 

Hence,  besides  the  diffuse  reticular  formation,  this  ventral 
part  of  the  bulb  contains  more  sharply  defined  collections  of 
grey  matter  in  the  olivary  nucleus,  and  the  other  bodies  just 
mentioned. 

§  610.  We  must  now  turn  to  the  dorsal  part  of  the  bulb. 
Here  in  the  first  place  we  .must  distinguish  between  the  portions 
of  grey  matter  which  are  more  immediately  connected  with  the 
cranial  nerves  taking  origin  from  this  part  of  the  bulb,  and  the 
portions  which  have  no  such  obvious  connection.  In  the  spinal 
cord,  the  anterior  horns  supply,  as  we  have  seen,  the  origins  of  the 
successive  anterior  motor  nerves ;  but  in  the  transformation  of 
the  cord  into  the  bulb  the  anterior  horns  have  been  broken  up 
or  displaced ;  and  the  parts  of  the  anterior  horns,  serving  as  the 
nuclei  of  origin  for  motor  nerves,  have  been  translated  from  the 
ventral  to  the  more  dorsal  regions.  Hence,  it  is  in  the  more 
dorsal  part  of  the  grey  matter  that  we  have  to  seek  for  the  nuclei 
of  origin  not  only  of  afferent  but  also  of  motor  cranial  nerves. 
It  will  be  convenient  to  consider  all  these  nuclei  of  origin  of 
cranial  nerves  by  themselves,  and  we  may  here  confine  ourselves 
to  the  grey  matter  of  other  nature.  We  may  however  say  that 
these  nuclei  from  that  of  the  third  nerve  backwards  are  more  or 
less  closely  associated  with  the  grey  matter  immediately  sur- 
rounding the  central  canal.  This  central  grey  matter,  in  the 
narrow  sense  of  the  term,  is  marked  out  somewhat  low  down 


CHAP.  IL]  THE  BRAIN.  947 

(Fig.  109,  3)  by  the  fibres  of  the  sensory  decussation  which  sweep 
round  it ;  it  appears  in  sections  higher  up  as  a  fairly  distinct 
region  (Fig.  109,  4) ;  and  it  is  this  part  of  the  grey  matter  which 
is  exposed  on  the  floor  of  the  fourth  ventricle  when  the  central 
canal  (Fig.  109,  5,  6)  opens  out  into  that  space.  We  say  exposed ; 
but  in  reality  the  true  grey  matter  is  covered  by  a  superficial 
layer  of  tissue  of  a  peculiar  nature  (indicated  in  fig.  109,  5,  6, 
by  a  thick  black  line)  similar  to  that  which  is  found  at  the  hind 
end  of  the  conus  medullaris  in  the  spinal  cord. 

We  saw  that  at  the  level  of  the  first  cervical  nerve  coincident 
with  the  horizontal  flattening  out  of  the  posterior  horns  the 
posterior  columns  assumed  very  large  dimensions.  In  this  region 
(Fig.  109,  1)  they  consist  entirely  of  white  matter,  that  is,  of 
longitudinal  fibres. 

At  a  little  higher  level,  however,  at  the  level  of  the  middle  of 
the  decussation  for  example,  an  islet  of  grey  matter  (Fig.  109, 
2,  ra.  p.  n.)  makes  its  appearance  in  the  median  posterior  column. 
A  little  further  forward,  at  the  level  of  the  established  pyramids, 
it  will  be  seen  (Fig.  109,  3)  that  this  islet  is  the  hind  end  of 
an  invasion  from  the  more  centrally  placed  grey  matter,  and  that 
at  the  same  time  there  has  taken  place  a  similar  inroad  of  grey 
matter  into  the  external  posterior  column  (Fig.  109,  3,  e.  p.  n.); 
indeed  a  slight  extension  of  grey  matter  into  the  external  pos- 
terior column  may  be  seen  even  before  this  (Fig.  109,  2,  e.  p.  n.). 
It  will  further  be  observed  that  these  grey  masses  have  so  largely 
encroached  on  the  white  matter,  that  both  the  median  posterior 
or  fasciculus  gracilis  and  the  external  posterior  column  or 
fasciculus  cuneatus,  instead  of  being  simply  tracts  of  white  fibres 
as  they  were  in  the  hinder  part  of  the  bulb  and  in  the  cord,  have 
now  become  columns  of  grey  matter  covered  by  a  relatively  thin 
layer  of  white  fibres.  These  columns  of  grey  matter  are  now 
called  respectively  the  median  posterior  nucleus,  or  nucleus 
fasciculi  gracilis,  or  more  shortly,  the  gracile  nucleus;  and  the 
external  posterior  nucleus,  or  nucleus  fasciculi  cuneati,  or  the 
cuneate  nucleus.  From  the  ventral  aspect  of  these  nuclei  a 
large  number  of  fibres  pass  ventrally  with  a  more  or  less 
curved  course  to  form  as  we  have  seen,  §  606,  the  superior  decus- 
sation and  to  pursue  certain  paths  through  the  reticular  formation, 
of  which  we  shall  speak  later  on.  It  is  at  this  level  and  for 
some  little  distance  above  (Fig.  109,  4,  5),  that  these  nuclei 
acquire  their  greatest  development.  Farther  forward  (Fig.  109, 
6),  when  the  fourth  ventricle  has  opened  out  and  the  nuclei 
of  the  cranial  nerves  are  becoming  conspicuous,  and  the  posterior 
columns  have  been  thrust  aside  laterally,  both  these  nuclei  have 
diminished  in  size ;  still  farther  forward  they  become  still  smaller, 
and  towards  the  pons  they  gradually  disappear. 

The  mass  of  gelatinous  substance,  forming  at  the  level  of 
the  first  cervical  nerve  the  swollen  caput  of  the  horn  close  to 


948 


THE   FIBRES   OF  THE   BULB. 


[BOOK  in. 


the  surface  but  separated  from  it  by  a  band  of  fibres  (Va)  of 
fine  calibre,  to  which  we  have  already  referred  as  belonging 
to  the  fifth  cranial  nerve,  increases  in  bulk  at  a  somewhat 
higher  level,  Fig.  109,  2,  3,  s.g.,  and  forms  on  the  surface  a 
slight  projection,  called  the  tubercle  of  Rolando.  It  soon,  how- 
ever, becomes  thrust  ventrally  by  the  divergence  of  the  posterior 
columns,  and  more  and  more  covered  up  by  the  fibres  which  are 
going  to  form  the  increasing  restiform  body,  Fig.  109,  4,  5,  6,  R. 
Retaining  this  position  the  islet  of  gelatinous  substance  diminishes 
in  size  farther  forwards,  Fig.  110,  s.g.,  and  eventually  disappears. 

§  611.  The  Fibres  of  the  Bulb.  It  is  obvious,  from  what  has 
already  been  said,  that  the  arrangement  into  posterior,  lateral  and 
anterior  columns,  so  clear  and  definite  in  the  spinal  cord,  becomes 


n.f.t. 


f!tL.i. 


FIG.  110.     THROUGH  THE  BULB  JUST  BEHIND  THE  PONS. 
Taken  in  the  line  110,  Fig.  108. 


(Sherrington.) 


Py.  Pyramids.  E.  Kestiform  Body.  Cbm.  cerebellum.  F.  Fillet.  /.  a.  e.  external, 
/.  a.  i.  internal  arcuate  fibres,  t.  bundle  of  fibres  from  olive  to  the  lenticular 
nucleus.  I.  posterior  longitudinal  bundles,  n.  f.  t.  nucleus  of  fasciculus  teres. 
s.  o.  superior  olive,  n.  c.  e.  nucleus  centralis  (the  marks  within  it  are  sections 
of  bundles  of  fibres  by  which  it  is  traversed),  s.  g.  substance  of  Eolando. 

V.  a.  ascending  root  of  fifth  nerve.  VII.  n.  nucleus  of  the  7th  nerve.  VIII. 
auditory  nerve,  chiefly  the  dorsal  or  cochlear  root ;  VIII.  a.  median  nucleus, 
VIII.  /3.  lateral  nucleus,  VIII.  7.  accessory  nucleus  of  auditory  nerve.  IX. 
fibres  of  root  of  ninth  nerve  passing  through  ascending  root  of  fifth  nerve. 

broken  up  in  the  bulb :  indeed  it  will  be  best  in  treating  of  the 
bulb,  not  to  attempt  to  trace  out  these  columns,  but  to  speak  of 
the  course  of  the  several  tracts  into  which  these  columns  may  be 
divided. 

The  direct  and  crossed  pyramidal  tracts  of  the  cord  unite  to 
form,  as  we  have  seen,  the  pyramid  of  the  bulb,  and  so  pass  on 
to  the  pons.  We  need  say  nothing  more  at  present  concerning 
this  important  pyramidal  strand  except  that,  as  we  trace  it  down 
from  the  pons  to  the  spinal  cord,  it  gives  off  to  the  bulb  itself 
fibres  which  make  connections  with  the  motor  fibres  of  the 
cranial  nerves  proceeding  from  this  region. 


CHAP,  ii.]  THE   BRAIN.  949 

Concerning  the  course  taken  by  the  other  less  conspicuous 
"  descending  "  tract,  the  antero-lateral  descending  tract,  our  know- 
ledge is  very  imperfect ;  nothing  definite  can  be  said  at  present. 

The  cerebellar  tract,  occupying  near  to  the  surface  a  position 
which  in  the  series  of  sections  (Fig.  109,  Gb)  appears  now  rather 
more  ventral  now  more  dorsal,  eventually  passes  into  the  restiform 
body,  of  which  it  forms  a  large  part,  and  thus  reaches  the  cere- 
bellum. The  antero-lateral  ascending  tract  possibly  also  takes  the 
same  course,  but  this  is  not  as  yet  certain. 

The  median  posterior  tract  or  column,  becoming  the  fasciculus 
gracilis,  ends  in  the  gracile  nucleus;  and  in  a  similar  manner 
the  external  posterior  column,  or  fasciculus  cuneatus,  ends  in  the 
median  and  lateral  masses  of  the  cuneate  nucleus.  As  we  have 
seen,  the  white  matter  of  these  columns  diminishes  as  the  nuclei 
increase ;  and  the  nuclei  after  absorbing,  so  to  speak,  the  white 
matter  diminish  in  turn ;  the  ascending  degeneration  observed  in 
these  columns  stops  at  these  nuclei.  It  is  a  suggestive  fact  that 
as  these  nuclei  diminish  forwards  the  restiform  body  increases  in 
bulk. 

The  remaining  fibres  of  the  cord,  belonging  partly  to  the 
anterior  column  and  partly  to  the  lateral  column,  not  gathered 
into  any  of  the  above  mentioned  tracts,  appear  to  end  chiefly  at 
all  events  in  the  reticular  formation  of  the  bulb  itself,  though 
some  are  carried  on  to  the  higher  parts  of  the  brain. 

§  612.  Thus  of  the  various  tracts  or  strands  of  the  spinal  cord 
two  only  are  known  definitely  and  certainly  to  pass  as  conspicuous 
unbroken  strands  through  the  bulb  to  or  from  higher  parts; 
namely,  the  pyramidal  tract  to  the  cerebrum  and  the  cerebellar 
tract  to  the  cerebellum.  All  or  nearly  all  the  rest  of  the  longitu- 
dinal fibres  of  the  cord  reaching  the  bulb  end,  as  far  as  we  know 
at  present,  in  some  part  or  other  of  the  bulb ;  and  we  may  infer 
that  some  or  other  nerve  cells  of  the  bulb  serve  as  relays  to 
connect  these  fibres  of  the  cord  with  other  parts  of  the  brain. 

The  gracile  and  cuneate  nuclei  stand  out  conspicuously  as 
relays  of  this  kind,  and  through  them  the  posterior  columns  of 
the  cord  make  secondary  connections  on  the  one  hand  with  the 
cerebellum  and  on  the  other  hand  with  various  regions  of  the 
cerebrum.  We  have  said  §  606  that  fibres  passing  ventrally  from 
the  gracile  and  cuneate  nuclei  sweep  in  a  curved  course  through 
the  reticular  formation  as  the  internal  arcuate  fibres  (Fig.  109, 
/.  a.  i.).  The  hindmost  of  these  form  the  superior  decussation 
already  referred  to  as  seen  in  sections  at  the  fore-part  of  and  in 
front  of  the  pyramidal  decussation  (Fig.  109,  3,  supra  Py.  dec.}. 
After  decussating  ventral  to  the  central  canal  these  fibres  form  an 
area  called  the  inter-olivary  layer  (Fig.  109,  4,  in.  ol.)  lying  dorsal 
to  the  pyramids  between  the  two  olivary  nuclei.  This  layer  may 
be  regarded  as  the  hind  end  or  beginning  on  each  side  of  a  remark- 
able longitudinal  strand  called  the  fillet  (Figs.  108,  B.F.,  110,  F.)y 


950  THE   FIBRES   OF  THE   BULB.  [BOOK  m. 

of  the  connections  of  which  in  the  front  part  of  the  brain  we  shall 
speak  hereafter.  Thus  these  two  nuclei  are  the  source  of  fibres 
which  cross  to  the  other  side  of  the  bulb,  and  reaching  the  inter- 
olivary  layer  dorsal  to  the  pyramids  run  up  to  higher  parts  of  the 
brain  by  the  fillet.  We  may  add  that  the  formation  of  the  fillet 
is  also  probably  assisted  by  fibres  from  a  tract  which  lies  just 
dorsal  to  the  interolivary  layer  and  is  derived  from  the  anterior 
columns  of  the  cord.  Besides  its  fibres  of  descending  degeneration 
the  anterior  column  contains  fibres  of  ascending  degeneration,  and 
these  rise  dorsally  in  the  bulb  to  form  the  tract  in  question. 
Though  the  whole  tract  is  of  some  length,  the  component  fibres 
appear  to  be  short. 

The  gracile  and  cuneate  nuclei  give  rise  also  to  other  fibres 
which,  though  also  sweeping  ventrally  and  crossing  to  the  other 
side,  do  not,  when  they  reach  the  inter-olivary  region,  assume  a 
longitudinal  direction  as  do  the  fibres  forming  the  fillet,  but  as 
external  arcuate  fibres  (Fig.  109, /.  a.  e.)  pursue  a  course  which  is 
at  first  ventral  along  the  side  of  the  anterior  fissure  and  then 
lateral  over  the  ventral  surface  of  the  pyramid  and  olivary  nucleus, 
by  which  path  they  reach  the  lateral  surface  of  the  bulb,  and  so 
the  restiform  body  and  cerebellum.  In  this  way,  the  two  nuclei 
in  question  contribute  to  the  restiform  body  of  the  opposite  side 
of  the  bulb.  These  external  arcuate  fibres,  which  as  they  sweep 
round  the  ventral  surface  of  the  pyramid  traverse  the  arcuate 
nucleus,  though  they  vary  much  in  individual  brains,  form  a 
considerable  portion  of  the  white  matter  seen  on  the  ventral  and 
lateral  surfaces  of  the  bulb ;  it  is  by  them  that  the  olivary  nucleus 
is  covered  up. 

The  cuneate  and  gracile  nuclei,  besides  this  crossed  and 
somewhat  roundabout  connection  with  the  restiform  body  of  the 
opposite  side,  are  believed  to  have  more  direct  connection  with 
the  restiform  body  of  the  same  side  by  means  of  fibres  which 
pass  by  a  more  or  less  direct  lateral  path  from  them  to  it. 
Accepting  this  view  we  may  say  that  the  two  nuclei  are  connected 
with  the  opposite  side  of  the  cerebellum  by  external  arcuate 
fibres,  and  with  the  same  side  of  the  cerebellum  by  the  other 
fibres  just  mentioned.  In  any  case  the  connection  between  the 
two  nuclei  and  the  cerebellum  is  large  and  important. 

Thus  the  important  strand  of  fibres  which  is  called  in  the  bulb 
the  restiform  body,  and  higher  up  the  inferior  peduncle  of  the 
cerebellum,  is  connected  with  the  spinal  cord  in  two  chief  ways : 
directly  by  means  of  the  cerebellar  tract,  and  indirectly  by  means 
of  the  cuneate  and  gracile  nuclei  which,  as  we  have  said,  diminish 
in  bulk  forwards  as  the  restiform  body  increases.  By  the  relay  of 
the  gracile  nucleus  it  is  brought  into  connection  with  the  median 
posterior  column  along  the  whole  length  of  the  cord,  and  so  with 
that  division  of  the  posterior  roots  which  (§  577)  in  each  of  the 
several  spinal  nerves  goes  to  form  that  column.  By  the  relay  of 


CHAP.  IL]  THE   BRAIN.  951 

the  cuneate  nucleus  it  is  brought  into  connection  with  such  parts 
of  the  external  posterior  column  as  end  in  that  nucleus,  and  thus 
probably  with  other  fibres  of  the  posterior  roots  of  the  upper 
spinal  nerves.  And  if  we  admit  that  the  cerebellar  tract  is 
connected,  by  the  relay  of  the  vesicular  cylinder  or  by  other  nerve 
cells,  with  the  rest  of  the  posterior  roots  of  the  spinal  nerves,  we 
may  conclude  that  the  restiform  body  is,  by  means  of  these  relays, 
a  prominent  continuation  of  all  the  spinal  posterior  roots. 

The  restiform  body  and  so  the  cerebellum  is  also  specially 
connected  with  the  olivary  body  of  the  opposite  side ;  for  when  in 
young  animals  one  side  of  the  cerebellum  is  removed  the  olivary 
body  of  the  opposite  side  atrophies.  The  course  of  the  fibres 
maintaining  this  connection  is  not  as  yet  accurately  known,  but 
they  probably  pass  from  the  olivary  nucleus  of  one  side  through 
the  interolivary  layer  and  so  laterally  through  the  reticular  forma- 
tion of  the  other  side.  Lastly  we  may  add  that  a  tract  which  is 
sometimes  included  in  the  restiform  body  as  its  median  or  inner 
division  has  quite  a  different  origin  from  any  of  the  above ;  the 
fibres  which  compose  it  come,  as  we  shall  see,  from  the  auditory 
nerve. 

The  further  connections  of  the  bulb  with  the  cerebrum  it 
will  be  best  to  leave  until  we  come  to  deal  with  the  structural 
arrangement  of  the  rest  of  the  brain. 

Meanwhile  enough  has  been  said  to  shew  that  the  bulb  differs 
very  materially  in  structure  from  the  spinal  cord.  The  grey  matter 
of  the  bulb  is  far  more  complex  in  its  nature  than  is  that  of  any 
part  of  the  cord ;  and  the  arrangement  of  the  several  strands  and 
tracts  of  fibres  is  far  more  intricate.  The  structural  features  on 
the  whole  perhaps  suggest  that  the  main  functions  of  the  bulb  are 
twofold ;  on  the  one  hand  it  seems  fitted  to  serve  as  a  head  centre 
governing  the  spinal  cord,  the  various  reins  of  which,  with  the 
exceptions  noted,  it  holds  as  it  were  in  its  hands;  on  the  other 
hand  it  appears  no  less  adapted  to  act  as  a  middleman  between 
parts  of  the  spinal  cord  below  and  various  regions  of  the  brain 
above.  As  we  shall  see  experiment  and  observation  give  support 
to  these  suggestions. 


SEC.     3.      THE      DISPOSITION     AND     CONNECTIONS     OF 
THE  GREY  AND  WHITE  MATTER  OF  THE  BRAIN. 

The  Grey  Matter. 

§  613.  As  we  pass  up  from  the  bulb  to  the  higher  parts  of  the 
brain,  the  differentiation  of  the  grey  matter  into  more  or  less 
separate  masses,  which  we  have  seen  begin  in  the  bulb,  becomes 
still  more  striking.  We  have  to  distinguish  a  large  number  of 
areas  or  collections  of  grey  matter  more  or  less  regular  in  form  and 
more  or  less  sharply  defined  from  the  surrounding  white  matter ; 
to  such  collections  the  several  terms  corpus,  locus,  nucleus  and 
the  like  have  from  time  to  time  been  given.  These  areas  or  col- 
lections vary  greatly  in  size,  in  form  and  in  histological  characters  ; 
they  differ  from  each  other  in  the  form,  size,  features  and  arrange- 
ment of  the  nerve  cells,  in  the  characters  of  the  nervous  network 
of  which  the  nerve  cells  form  a  part,  and  especially  perhaps  in  the 
extent  to  which  the  more  distinctly  grey  matter  is  traversed  and 
broken  up  by  bundles  of  white  fibres.  Guided  by  the  analogy  of  the 
spinal  cord,  as  well  as  by  the  results  of  experiments  and  observa- 
tions directed  to  the  brain  itself,  we  are  led  to  believe  that  the 
complex  functions  of  the  brain  are  intimately  associated  with  this 
grey  matter ;  and  a  full  knowledge  of  the  working  of  the  brain  will 
carry  with  it  a  knowledge  of  the  nature  and  meaning  of  the 
intricate  arrangement  of  the  cerebral  grey  matter.  At  present, 
however,  our  ignorance  as  to  these  things  is  great ;  and,  though 
various  theoretical  classifications  of  the  several  collections  of  grey 
matter  have  been  proposed,  it  will  perhaps  be  wisest  to  content 
ourselves  here  with  a  very  broad  and  simple  arrangement.  We 
will  divide  the  whole  grey  matter  of  the  brain  into  four  categories 
only.  1.  The  central  grey  matter  lining  the  neural  canal ;  and 
with  this  we  may  consider  the  nuclei  of  the  cranial  nerves  some  of 
which  are  closely  associated  with  it.  2.  The  superficial  grey 
matter  of  the  roof  of  some  of  the  main  divisions  of  the  brain,  such 
as  that  of  the  cerebral  hemispheres,  and  of  the  cerebellum.  3. 
The  intermediate  grey  matter  more  or  less  closely  connected  with 


CHAP.  IL]  THE   BRAIN.  953 

the  crura  cerebri.  4.  Other  collections  and  areas  of  grey  matter. 
We  will,  moreover,  confine  ourselves  at  present  for  the  most  part 
to  their  general  features  and  topography,  reserving  what  we 
have  to  say  concerning  their  histological  characters  for  another 
occasion. 


1.     The  Central  Grey  Matter,  and  the  Nuclei  of  the  Cranial 

Nerves. 

§  614.  The  ventricles  of  the  brain  like  the  central  canal  of  the 
spinal  cord,  of  which  they  are  a  continuation,  are  lined  by  an  epi- 
thelium which  is  in  general  a  single  layer  of  columnar  cells  said  to 
be  ciliated  throughout,  though  it  is  often  difficult  to  demonstrate 
the  cilia.  Beneath  this  epithelium  lies  a  layer  of  somewhat  pecu- 
liar neuroglia,  forming  with  the  epithelium,  as  we  have  said 
(§  610),  the  ependyma,  which,  well  developed  in  the  floor  of  the 
fourth  ventricle  and  in  the  walls  of  the  third  ventricle,  and  of  the 
aqueduct,  is  thin  and  scanty  in  the  lateral  ventricles.  Beneath, 
and  more  or  less  connected  with  the  ependyma  in  the  sides  and 
floor  of  the  third  ventricle,  is  a  fairly  conspicuous  layer  of  grey 
matter,  which  is  well  developed  in  the  parts  of  the  floor  exposed 
on  the  ventral  surface  of  the  brain,  and  known  as  the  lamina  ter- 
minalis,  the  anterior  and  posterior  perforated  spaces,  the  tuber 
cinereum  &c.  This  layer  is  not  continued  forwards  into  the  lateral 
ventricles  of  the  cerebral  hemispheres,  but  it  is  well-developed 
backwards  along  the  aqueduct  (Figs.  113,  114)  and  in  the  floor  of 
the  fourth  ventricle,  and  through  the  bulb  becomes,  as  we  have 
seen  (§  610),  continuous  with  the  central  grey  matter  of  the  cord. 
The  nerve  cells  of  this  grey  matter  are  on  the  whole  small  and  in 
many  places  scant. 

§  615.  The  several  roots  of  the  cranial  nerves  from  the  third 
nerve  backwards  may  be  traced  within  the  brain  substance  to 
special  collections  of  grey  matter,  called  the  nuclei  of  the  cranial 
nerves,  some  of  which  lie  close  upon  the  central  grey  matter, 
while  others  are  placed  at  some  distance  from  it.  The  optic 
nerve  and  what  is  sometimes  called  the  olfactory  nerve,  namely, 
the  olfactory  bulb  and  tract,  may  advantageously  be  dealt  with 
apart,  since  these  two  nerves  are  not,  like  the  other  cranial  nerves, 
simple  outgrowths  from  the  walls  of  the  original  neural  canal,  but 
are  in  reality  elongated  vesicles,  budded  off  from  the  neural 
canal,  the  cavities  of  which  have  been  obliterated.  We  may  add 
that  part  of  the  retina,  and  of  the  grey  matter  of  the  olfactory 
tract,  may  perhaps  be  considered  as  corresponding  to  the  nuclei 
of  which  we  are  speaking,  the  retinal  and  proper  olfactory  fibres 
being  connected  with  them  very  much  as  the  fibres  of  the  re- 
maining cranial  nerves  are  connected  with  their  respective  nuclei. 


954  NUCLEI   OF   CRANIAL   NERVES.          [BOOK  m. 

In  the  brain,  the  segmental  regularity  of  the  nerve  roots  so 
conspicuous  in  the  spinal  cord  is  very  greatly  obscured.  We  shall 
have  something  to  say  on  this  point  later  on ;  but  at  present  we 
may  be  content  to  treat  the  several  nerves  in  a  simple  topographical 
manner.  They  maybe  seen  in  a  ventral  view  of  the  brain  Fig.  108,  C 
leaving  the  brain  at  various  levels  by  what  is  called  their  "  super- 
ficial origin ; "  the  third  nerve  issuing  in  front  of  the  pons,  and 
the  last  or  hypoglossal  stretching  back  to  the  hind  end  of  the  bulb. 
Part,  indeed,  of  the  eleventh  nerve,  the  spinal  accessory  nerve 
properly  so  called,  makes  connections  with  the  spinal  cord  below 
the  bulb  as  far  back  as  the  sixth  or  seventh  cervical  nerve,  or  even 
lower;  but  this  part  may  by  these  connections  be  distinguished 
from  the  remaining  part  of  the  nerve,  as  well  as  from  all  the  other 
cranial  nerves.  The  nuclei  to  which  the  nerve  roots  may  be  traced 
within  the  brain  substance,  sometimes  spoken  of  as  the  "  deep 
origin,"  range  in  position  from  the  hinder  part  of  the  bulb  to  the 
hind  end  of  the  third  ventricle.  The  nucleus  of  the  hypoglossal 
nerve  begins  in  the  bulb  just  above  the  decussation  of  the  pyra- 
mids, the  nucleus  of  the  third  nerve  ends  beneath  the  hind  end  of 
the  floor  of  the  third  ventricle ;  and  all  the  rest  of  the  nuclei  may 
be  broadly  described  as  placed  between  these  limits  in  various 
parts  of  the  floor  of  the  central  canal  or  in  adjoining  structures, 
though  part  of  one  nucleus,  namely,  that  of  the  fifth  nerve,  can 
be  traced,  as  we  shall  see,  back  into  the  spinal  cord  as  far  as  the 
second  cervical  nerve  and  probably  extends  still  farther.  Fig.  115 
is  a  diagram  shewing  in  a  roughly  approximate  manner  the  nuclei 
of  the  several  nerves  as  they  would  appear  in  a  bird's-eye  view  of 
the  floor  of  the  aqueduct  and  fourth  ventricle  looked  at  on  the 
dorsal  aspect. 

§  616.  The  Twelfth  or  Hypoglossal  Nerve.  The  nucleus  of  this 
nerve,  which  it  will  be  convenient  to  take  first  (Fig.  115,  xn.),  is 
a  long  column  of  grey  matter  lying  in  the  bulb  parallel  to,  and 
very  close  to,  the  median  line.  It  reaches  from  the  hinder  part 
of  the  fourth  ventricle,  at  about  the  level  of  the  hind  end  of  the 
auditory  nucleus,  as  far  back  as  beyond  the  hind  end  of  the  olivary 
body.  At  its  extreme  hind  end  or  beginning  (Fig.  109,  2),  it 
occupies  a  ventral  position  and  is  a  part  of  the  anterior  horn ; 
thence  it  gradually  rises  dorsally  (Fig.  109,  3,  4,  5),  but  so  long  as 
the  central  canal  remains  closed  continues  to  occupy  a  distinctly 
ventral  position  in  reference  to  the  central  canal ;  in  its  front  part, 
it  is,  by  the  opening  up  of  the  fourth  ventricle,  brought  into  an 
apparently  more  dorsal  position  (Fig.  109,  6). 

The  nucleus  consists  mainly  of  large  nerve  cells  with  distinct 
axis-cylinder  processes,  which  though  pursuing  a  somewhat  irre- 
gular course  may  be  traced  into  the  fibres  of  the  nerve.  These, 
starting  from  the  ventral  surface  of  the  nucleus  along  its  length,  run 
ventrally  through  the  reticular  formation,  and  making  their  way  in 
a  series  of  bundles,  between  the  olivary  nucleus  on  the  lateral  side 


CHAP,  ii.]  THE   BRAIN.  955 

and  the  pyramid  and  median  accessory  olive  on  the  median  side, 
gain  the  surface  along  the  groove  which  separates  the  pyramid 
from  the  olivary  body. 

§  617.  The  Ninth  or  Glossopharyngeal,  Tenth  or  Vagus,  and 
Eleventh  or  Spinal  accessory  Nerves.  It  will  be  advantageous 
to  consider  these  three  nerves  together. 

In  the  spinal  accessory  nerve  we  must  distinguish,  as  we  have 
said,  two  parts:  the  "spinal  accessory"  proper,  formed  by  the 
roots  which  come  off  from  the  cervical  spinal  cord,  reaching  as  far 
down  as  the  sixth  or  seventh  cervical  nerve,  and  the  "  bulbar 
accessory,"  whose  roots  come  off  from  the  bulb  just  below  the 
vagus. 

The  spinal  accessory  proper  takes  origin  in  the  group  of  cells 
lying  in  the  extreme  lateral  margin  of  the  anterior  horn,  from 
whence  the  fibres  proceed  directly  outwards  through  the  lateral 
column,  and  issue  from  the  cord  along  a  line  intermediate  between 
the  anterior  and  posterior  roots ;  the  upper  roots  undergo,  with  the 
portion  of  the  lateral  horn  from  which  they  spring,  the  shifting 
spoken  of  in  §  605. 

The  bulbar  accessory  starts  from  an  elongated  nucleus  in  the 
bulb  which  is  common  to  it,  to  the  vagus,  and  to  the  glosso-pharyn- 
geal;  hence  we  have  taken  these  three  nerves  together.  This 
(Fig.  115)  stretches  farther  forward  than  the  hypoglossal  nucleus, 
reaching  the  level  of  the  transverse  fibres  called  striae  acusticae 
(sir.),  but  does  not  extend  so  far  behind. 

In  transverse  sections  of  the  bulb,  which  pass  a  little  below 
and  a  little  above  the  point  of  the  calamus  scriptorius  (Fig.  109, 
4,  5),  two  nuclei  or  collections  of  cells  are  seen  in  the  grey 
matter  round  the  central  canal.  The  more  ventral  one  is  the 
hypoglossal  nucleus,  the  more  dorsal  one  the  beginning  or  hind 
part  of  the  combined  accessory- vago-glossopharyngeal  nucleus. 

When  a  little  farther  forward  the  central  canal  opens  out 
into  the  fourth  ventricle  (by  which  change  the  hypoglossal  nucleus 
(Fig.  109,  6  n.  XII.)  is  brought  nearer  to  the  dorsal  surface  in  the 
floor  of  the  fourth  ventricle)  this  combined  nucleus,  increasing  in 
breadth,  is  thrown  to  the  side  and  assumes  a  more  lateral  position, 
lying  now  on  the  side  of,  but  still  somewhat  dorsal  to,  the  hypo- 
glossal  nucleus,  between  it  and  the  now  diminishing  gracile 
nucleus.  In  this  position  the  nucleus  appears  to  consist  of  two 
parts,  a  median  and  lateral,  the  median  part  having  conspicuous 
nerve  cells  of  moderate  size,  the  lateral  part  having  but  few  cells, 
and  those  of  small  size.  From  this  level  the  nucleus  runs 
forwards,  maintaining  nearly  the  same  position  in  the  floor  of  the 
fourth  ventricle  but  gradually  becoming  thinner,  and  ends  as  we 
have  said  at  about  the  level  of  the  striae  acusticae  on  the  dorsal 
surface  corresponding  on  the  ventral  surface  to  a  level  a  little 
behind  the  hind  margin  of  the  pons. 

From  this  combined  nucleus,  but   chiefly  from   the   median 


956  NUCLEI   OF   CRANIAL   NERVES.          [BOOK  in. 

part,  fibres  sweep  in  a  ventral  and  lateral  direction  through  the 
dorsal  part  of  the  reticular  formation,  pass  ventral  to,  or  in  some 
cases  through  the  gelatinous  substance  and  the  strand  of  fibres 
connected  with  the  fifth  nerve  (Fig.  109  v.  a),  and  reach  the  surface 
of  the  bulb  on  its  lateral  aspect  in  a  line  between  the  olivary  and 
restiform  bodies  (Fig.  108,  c).  Along  this  line  may  be  seen  (Fig. 

108,  c.)  a  series  of  roots ;  of  these  the  lowest,  the  accessory  roots, 
spring  from  the  hind  part,  the  highest,  the  glossopharyngeal  roots, 
from  the  front  part  (and  it  is  these  especially  which  pierce  the 
gelatinous  substance  (Fig.  110,  IX.  a)),  and  the  intermediate,  the 
vagus  roots,  from  the  middle  part  of  the  combined  nucleus.    Hence 
we  may  speak  of  the  hind  part  of  the  whole  nucleus  as  being  the 
accessory  nucleus,  the  middle  part  as  the  vagus  nucleus,  and  the 
front  part  as  the  glossopharyngeal  nucleus. 

All  the  fibres  however  of  the  roots  of  these  three  nerves  do  not 
take  origin  from  the  nucleus  in  question ;  some  of  the  fibres  start 
in  a  different  way.  In  sections  of  the  bulb  above  the  decussation 
of  the  pyramid  a  patch  of  grey  matter  is  seen  lying  in  the  lateral 
part  of  the  reticular  formation  (Fig.  109,  x.  m),  about  midway 
between  the  ventral  and  dorsal  surfaces.  What  is  thus  disclosed 
by  sections  is  a  column  of  grey  matter,  the  "  nucleus  ambiguus  " 
(Fig.  115,  na),  stretching  about  as  far  forwards  and  backwards  as 
the  combined  accessory-vago-glossopharyngeal  nucleus,  but  placed 
distinctly  more  ventrally  and  somewhat  more  laterally.  (In  Fig. 
115,  it  and  the  combined  nucleus  are  represented  on  different 
sides  of  the  diagram,  to  avoid  confusion  through  the  overlapping 
of  the  shading.)  From  it  fibres  curve  round  (Fig.  109,  6,  x.  m),  to 
join  the  accessory- vago-glossopharyngeal  roots,  but  especially  the 
vagus  roots.  It  may  therefore  be  considered  as  a  second  nucleus 
of  the  vagus  (and  possibly  of  the  other)  roots. 

But  there  is  yet  a  third  source  of  some  of  the  fibres  of  the 
nerves  of  which  we  are  speaking.  In  sections  through  the  bulb 
there  may  be  seen  just  ventral  to  and  a  little  lateral  to  the 
combined  nucleus  (Fig.  109,  4,  5,  6,  IX.  a),  the  circular  section 
of  a  longitudinal  bundle  of  fibres.  In  the  hinder  sections  (Fig. 

109,  4)  the  bundle  is  a  very  thin  one  and  still  further  back  it 
is  lost  to  view,  though  there  are  reasons  for  thinking  that  some 
of  the  fibres  are  continued  back  into  the  cervical  cord,  as  far  as 
the  origin  of  the  fourth  cervical  nerve  or  even  beyond ;   in  the 
more  forward  sections  (Fig.  109,  5  and  6),  it  increases  in  diameter 
and  may  be  traced  forward   to  the   front  end   of  the   combined 
nucleus  into  which  it  merges.     It  is  a  bundle  of  fibres  which, 
starting  successively  in  the  lateral  grey  matter  of  the  cervical  cord 
and  higher  up  in  the  reticular  formation  of  the  bulb,  run  longi- 
tudinally forwards;   the  bundle  at  first  increases  in  size  by  the 
addition  of  fresh  fibres  at  each  step;   but  farther  forwards  the 
fibres   leave   the   bundle   to   pass  into   the  roots  of  the   nerves 
of  which  we   are   speaking,  especially   of  the   glossopharyngeal, 


CHAP.  IL]  THE   BRAIN.  957 

and   the   bundle   eventually  ends    in  front  by  passing  into  the 

flossopharyngeal  roots.  The  grey  matter  from  which  these 
bres  take  origin  does  not  form  a  denned  compact  area,  is  not 
therefore  a  nucleus  in  the  sense  in  which  we  are  now  using  the 
term,  but  is  diffused  among  the  rest  of  the  grey  matter  along  a 
considerable  length.  The  fibres  are  nevertheless  fibres  of  nerve 
roots,  and  the  bundle  is  called  the  ascending  root  of  the 
glossopharyngeal,  the  term  ascending  being  used  since  it  is 
customary  to  trace  such  structures  from  below  upwards,  that  is 
from  behind  forwards;  though  since  the  fibres  in  question  are 
probably  afferent  fibres  carrying  impulses  backwards  from  the 
nerves  to  the  grey  matter,  'descending'  would  be  the  more 
appropriate  word.  The  bundle  has  also  been  called  the  fasciculus 
solitarius  ;  and,  since  its  position  has  been  supposed  to  correspond 
to  that  of  the  area  marked  out  experimentally  as  the  respiratory 
centre,  §  361,  it  has  been  spoken  of  as  the  respiratory  bundle. 

The  roots  of  these  three  nerves  then,  the  bulbar  accessory,  the 
vagus,  and  the  glossopharyngeal,  all  leaving  the  surface  of  the 
brain  along  the  line  between  the  olive  and  the  restiform  body, 
and  all  so  far  alike  that  it  is  impossible  upon  mere  inspection  to 
say  where  in  the  series  the  fibres  of  the  middle  nerve,  the  vagus, 
begin  and  end,  spring  from  three  sources,  the  combined  nucleus, 
the  nucleus  ambiguus,  and  the  ascending  root. 

§  618.  The  Eighth  or  Auditory  Nerve.  This  nerve  differs  from 
the  other  nerves  which  we  are  now  considering  in  being  a  nerve  of 
special  sense  ;  its  arrangements  are  complicated.  In  a  view  of  the 
base  of  the  brain  (Fig.  108,  (7.),  the  nerve  is  seen  to  leave  the 
surface  of  the  brain  from  the  ventral  surface  of  the  fore  part  of 
the  restiform  body  at  the  hind  margin  of  the  pons  as  two  strands 
or  roots,  one  of  which  winds  round  the  restiform  body  so  as  to 
reach  its  dorsal  surface  while  the  other  appears  to  sink  into  the 
substance  of  the  bulb  to  the  median  side  of  the  restiform  body ; 
and  in  a  transverse  section  of  the  bulb  (Fig.  110)  just  behind  the 
pons  the  two  roots  may  be  seen  embracing  the  restiform  body,  one 
passing  on  its  dorsal  and  the  other  on  its  ventral  side.  The  former 
is  called  the  dorsal  root  (Fig.  110),  or  sometimes  the  lateral  root, 
or  since  it  reaches  farther  back  or  lower  down  than  the  other,  the 
posterior  or  inferior  root ;  the  latter  is  called  the  ventral  root  (Fig. 
Ill),  or  sometimes  the  median  root,  or  since  it  reaches  farther 
forward  or  higher  up  than  the  other,  the  anterior  or  superior  root. 
When  we  come  to  study  the  ear  we  shall  find  that  one  division  of 
the  auditory  nerve  is  distributed  to  the  cochlea  alone  and  is  called 
the  nervous  cochlearis,  the  rest  of  the  nerve  being  distributed  to  the 
utricle,  saccule  and  semicircular  canals  as  the  nervus  vestibularis. 
As  we  shall  see,  there  are  reasons  for  thinking  that  the  vestibular 
nerve  carries  up  to  the  brain  from  the  semicircular  canals  impulses 
other  than  those  or  besides  those  which  give  rise  to  sensations  of 
sound,  whereas  the  cochlear  nerve  appears  to  be  exclusively  con- 


958  NUCLEI   OF   CRANIAL   NERVES.          [BOOK  in. 

cerned  in  hearing;  and  in  some  structural  details  these  two 
divisions  of  the  auditory  nerve  differ  from  each  other.  Hence 
it  is  important  to  note  that  according  to  careful  investigations  the 
cochlear  nerve  is  the  continuation  of  the  dorsal  root  and  the 
vestibular  nerve  the  continuation  of  the  ventral  root. 


FIG.  111.     THKOUGH  THE  WIDEST  PAKT  OF  THE  FOURTH  VENTRICLE.     (Sherrington.) 
Taken  in  the  line  111.     Fig.  108. 

Py.  Pyramidal  fibres  cut  transversely,  tr.  P.  the  superficial  (ventral)  transverse 
fibres  of  the  pons.  The  shaded  part  of  the  pons  (gr.  P.)  indicates  grey  matter 
mingled  with  the  deeper  transverse  fibres.  F.  the  fillet.  Tp.  the  trapezium. 
C.  E.  the  restiform  body  or  inferior  peduncle  of  the  cerebellum,  cut  across 
obliquely.  S.  P.  the  superior  peduncles  of  the  cerebellum.  r.  raphe. 
s.  o.  superior  olive.  C.  D.  corpus  dentatum  of  the  cerebellum.  Rf.  n.  the 
nucleus  of  the  roof.  s.  g.  tubercle  of  Kolando.  V.  S.  section  through  sulcus  in 
the  vermis  superior  of  the  cerebellum,  t.  bundle  from  the  olive  to  the 
lenticular  nucleus. 

VIII.  the  eighth  or  auditory  nerve,  its  ventral  or  vestibular  root,  proceeding  from 
VIII.  £.  the  front  part  of  the  lateral  auditory  nucleus.  VII.  n.  the  nucleus 
of  the  seventh  or  facial  nerve.  VI.  the  nucleus  of  the  sixth  nerve.  VII.  g. 
fibres  of  the  seventh  nerve  cut  across  as  they  sweep  round  the  nucleus  of  the 
sixth  before  issuing  from  the  pons  as  VII. 

4th.  the  fourth  ventricle,  here  roofed  in  by  the  cerebellum;   the  shading  of  the 

central  grey  matter  immediately   surrounding   the  ventricle  is,  for  the  sake  of 

simplicity,  omitted. 


CHAP,  ii.]  THE   BRAIN.  959 

With  these  roots  of  the  auditory  nerve  proper  also  issues,  a 
little  in  front  of  the  ventral  root,  the  small  nerve  called  the  portio 
intermedia  Wrisbergi,  which  goes  to  join  the  facial  nerve. 

The  auditory  nucleus,  as  a  whole,  is  a  broad  mass,  having  in 
transverse  sections  of  the  bulb  a  somewhat  triangular  form,  lying 
in  the  lateral  parts  of  the  floor  of  the  fourth  ventricle,  reaching 
in  front  somewhat  beyond  the  level  of  the  striae  acusticae,  and 
overlapping  behind  the  front  parts  of  the  nucleus  ambiguus  and 
the  combined  accessory- vago-glossopharyngeal  nucleus ;  it  extends 
laterally  some  distance  outside  the  former  nucleus. 

The  nucleus  however  consists  of  two  distinct  parts,  a  median 
or  inner  nucleus  (Fig.  115,  VIII.  m.),  characterized  by  the  presence 
of  small  cells,  and  a  lateral  or  outer  nucleus  (Fig.  115,  VIII.  I.), 
the  cells  of  which  are  much  larger,  some  of  them  being  very  large. 
The  lateral  nucleus  is  placed  somewhat  deeper  than,  ventral  to, 
the  median  nucleus;  it  also  extends  farther  forwards  (Figs.  110 
and  111,  VIII.  /3),  so  that  the  front  end  of  the  whole  nucleus  is 
furnished  by  the  lateral  nucleus  alone  which  at  its  front  end 
occupies  a  more  dorsal  position  than  at  its  hind  end. 

Moreover  this  auditory  nucleus  thus  placed  in  the  floor  of  the 
fourth  ventricle  is  not  the  whole  of  the  nucleus  of  the  auditory 
nerve.  At  the  convergence  of  the  dorsal  and  ventral  roots  on 
the  ventral  surface  of  the  restiform  body  is  placed  a  group  of  cells, 
forming  a  swelling  which  in  its  general  appearance  and  in  the 
characters  of  its  cells  is  not  unlike  a  ganglion  on  the  posterior 
root  of  a  spinal  nerve.  This  is  called  the  accessory  nucleus. 

When  we  trace  the  fibres  of  the  nerve  centralwards  into  the 
brain,  we  find  that  a  large  number  at  least  of  the  fibres  of  the 
dorsal  root,  cochlear  nerve  (Fig.  110),  end,  according  to  most 
observers,  in  the  cells  of  the  accessory  nucleus,  or  in  nerve  cells 
lying  dorsal  to  the  accessory  nucleus  and  especially  in  a  group  of 
cells  giving  rise  to  the  tuber culum  acusticum,  which,  small  in  man, 
is  conspicuous  in  some  animals.  Hence  the  farther  part  of  this 
dorsal  root  as  it  winds  round  the  lateral  and  dorsal  surface  of  the 
restiform  body,  consists  largely,  if  not  wholly,  of  fibres  which  are 
derived  not  directly  from  the  trunk  of  the  nerve,  but  indirectly 
through  the  relay  of  the  accessory  nucleus  or  of  other  cells. 
Reaching  the  dorsal  surface  of  the  restiform  body,  these  fibres 
appear  on  the  floor  of  the  fourth  ventricle  as  the  striae  acusticae 
(Fig.  108,  sir),  and  end  partly  in  the  median  nucleus,  partly  in 
other  regions  of  the  bulb.  The  exact  determination,  however,  of 
the  endings  of  this  root  is  a  matter  of  considerable  difficulty ; 
some  observers  regard  the  accessory  nucleus  as  homologous,  not 
with  the  Gasserian  and  ,with  the  spinal  ganglia,  but  with  the 
other,  true,  cranial  nuclei ;  and  in  any  case  we  must  probably 
consider  the  median  division  of  the  auditory  nucleus,  not  as  a 
nucleus  in  the  sense  in  which  we  are  now  using  it,  but  rather  as 
a  secondary  connection  within  the  bulb. 

F.  61 


960  NUCLEI   OF   CRANIAL   NERVES.         [BOOK  in. 

When  we  trace  the  ventral  root,  vestibular  nerve  (Fig.  Ill), 
inwards  we  find  that  making,  according  to  most  observers,  no 
connections  at  all  with  the  accessory  nucleus,  it  passes  (Fig.  Ill, 
viii.)  to  the  median  side  of  the  restiform  body,  between  it  and 
the  ascending  root  of  the  fifth  nerve,  and  so  reaches  the  lateral 
division  of  the  nucleus,  in  the  large  cells  of  which  most  at  least 
of  its  fibres  are  said  to  end  and  which  therefore  may  be  regarded 
as  the  nucleus  of  the  ventral  root.  On  this  point  however  all 
authors  are  not  agreed.  The  lateral  auditory  nucleus,  with  the 
fibres  proceeding  to  and  from  it,  lying  as  they  do  to  the  median 
or  inner  side  of  the  restiform  body  proper,  are  sometimes  spoken 
of  as  the  median  or  inner  division  of  the  restiform  body;  and 
from  the  nucleus  a  considerable  number  of  fibres  pass  up  with 
the  restiform  body  into  the  cerebellum  as  a  continuation  of  this 
"  median  division  of  the  restiform  body."  Some  authors  maintain 
that  these  fibres  are  continued  straight  on  from  the  nerve  to  the 
cerebellum ;  but  the  more  recent  investigations  seem  to  shew  that 
they  all  make  connections  with  the  nerve  cells  of  the  lateral  nucleus 
on  their  way.  These  fibres  constitute  a  connection  between  the 
auditory  (vestibular)  nerve  and  the  cerebellum,  the  physiological 
significance  of  which  we  shall  see  later  on ;  we  may  perhaps 
compare  it  to  the  connection  between  the  posterior  roots  of  the 
spinal  nerves  and  the  cerebellum  through  (the  vesicular  cylinder 
and)  the  cerebellar  tract. 

The  other  central  connections  of  the  lateral  nucleus  are,  like 
those  of  the  accessory  and  of  the  median  nucleus,  complicated  and 
obscure.  But  we  may  call  attention  to  a  set  of  fibres  which, 
starting  apparently  in  the  accessory  nucleus,  run  directly  trans- 
verse in  the  ventral  region  of  the  tegmentum  just  dorsal  to  the 
transverse  fibres  of  the  pons,  forming  what  is  called  the  trapezium 
(Fig.  Ill,  Tp.). 

Lastly,  we  may  add  that  the  fibres  of  the  peculiar  portio 
intermedia  appear  to  take  origin  from  the  accessory  nucleus. 

§  619.  The  Seventh  or  Facial  Nerve.  The  nucleus  (Fig.  115, 
VII.  and  Figs.  110,  111,  VII.  ??.),  of  this  nerve  (it  being 
borne  in  mind  that  the  motor  fibres  for  the  orbital  region  (the 
orbicular  muscle  &c.),  though  they  run  in  the  trunk  of  this  nerve, 
really  belong  to  the  third  nerve  and  take  origin  from  the  hind 
part  of  the  nucleus  of  the  third  nerve)  narrower  in  front  than 
behind,  reaches  from  the  level  of  the  striae  acusticae  some 
distance  into  the  region  of  the  pons,  and  occupies  in  the  midst 
of  the  reticular  formation,  a  little  dorsal  of  the  patch  of  grey 
matter  called  the  upper  olive,  a  position  corresponding  closely  to 
that  of  the  nucleus  ambiguus.  The  cells  of  the  nucleus  are  large, 
and  possess  well-marked  axis-cylinder  processes,  which  are  gathered 
up  at  the  dorsal  surface  of  the  nucleus  to  form  the  root.  This, 
rising  up  dorsally,  describes  a  loop  (Fig.  Ill,  VII.  g.)  round  the 
nucleus  of  the  sixth  or  abducens  nerve,  running  forward  for  some 


.CHAP.  IL] 


THE   BRAIN. 


961 


little  distance  dorsal  to  that  nucleus,  and  then  descends  again 
ventrally,  passing  to  the  lateral  side  of  its  own  nucleus,  between 
it  and  the  ascending  root  of  the  fifth  ( Fa) ;  it  thus  gains  the 
surface  of  the  brain  at  the  hinder  margin  of  the  pons,  lateral  to 


FIG.  112.     THBOUGH  THE  PONS  AT  THE  EXIT  OF  THE  FIFTH 
NEBVE.     (Sherrington.) 

(In  the  line  112,  Fig.  108.) 

C.  R.  Kemains  of  restiform  body.  S.  P.  superior  peduncle  of  the  cerebellum. 
F.  m.  median,  F.  I.  lateral  Fillet.  T.  E.  tegmental  reticular  formation. 
tr.  P.  superficial  transverse  fibres  of  the  Pons.  I.  posterior  longitudinal 
bundles.  V.  s.  superior  vermix  ;  sections  of  three  folia  are  shewn,  one  being 
detached;  between  them  the  intervening  sulci  laid  open  by  the  section  are 
seen.  VI.  a.  valve  of  Vieussens  or  anterior  velum,  r.'raphe.  Py.  Pyramidal 
fibres,  gr.  P.  grey  matter  of  the  Pons.  s.  o.  superior  olive.  t.  placed  on 
the  left  side  indicates  the  position  of  a  bundle  of  longitudinal  fibres  which 
may  be  traced  forward  into  the  subthalamic  regions.  V.  m.  motor  nucleus, 
V.  s.  sensory  nucleus,  and  7.  roots  of  the  fifth  nerve. 

4th,  fourth  ventricle;  shading  of  central  grey  matter  omitted  as  in  Fig.  111. 

the  abducens,  opposite  the  front  end  of  the  groove  between  the 
olivary  body  and  the  restiform  body.  As  it  thus  encircles  the 
nucleus  of  the  abducens,  it  looks  as  if  it  were  receiving  fibres 
from  that  body ;  but  the  evidence  goes  to  shew  that  these  fibres 

61—2 


962  NUCLEI   OF   CRANIAL  NERVES.          [BOOK  in. 

simply  pass  through  the  nucleus,  and  do  not  take  origin  from  any 
of  its  cells. 

§  620.  The  Sixth  or  Abducens  Nerve.  This  nerve  starts  from  a 
compact  oval  nucleus  (Fig.  115,  VI.),  lying  at  the  level  of  the 
hinder  part  of  the  pons,  and  therefore  of  the  front  part  of  the 
fourth  ventricle,  in  the  central  grey  matter  of  the  floor  of  the 
ventricle,  or  rather  just  between  it  and  the  reticular  formation,  a 
little  on  one  side  of  the  median  line  (Fig.  Ill,  VI.).  A  slight 
swelling  of  the  floor  of  the  fourth  ventricle,  eminentia  teres, 
marks  its  position  (Fig.  115,  e.  t).  The  nucleus  contains  fairly 
large  nerve  cells,  with  distinct  axis-cylinder  processes.  These  are 
gathered  at  the  median  side  of  the  nucleus  to  form  the  thin 
root,  which  passing  ventrally  and  laterally,  at  some  little  distance 
from  the  median  raphe,  through  the  reticular  formation,  runs 
backward  above  the  pyramidal  bundles  of  the  pons,  and  finally 
comes  to  the  surface  at  the  hinder  edge  of  the  pons,  opposite  the 
front  end  of  the  pyramid  (Fig.  108,  O). 

§  621.  The  Fifth  or  Trigeminal  Nerve.  This  nerve,  as  it  comes 
to  the  surface  on  the  ventral  aspect  of  the  pons  (Fig.  108,  C.), 
near  the  front  edge,  at  some  distance  from  the  median  line, 
consists  of  two  parts,  a  smaller  motor  root  and  a  larger  sensory 
root,  the  latter  bearing  the  large  ganglion  of  Gasser;  and  the 
origin  of  the  nerve  is  in  many  ways  complex.  Both  roots  may 
be  traced  in  an  oblique  direction  (Fig.  112,  V.)  inwards  and 
towards  the  dorsal  surface,  through  the  pons  to  the  reticular 
formation  beneath  the  floor  of  the  front  part  of  the  fourth 
ventricle,  the  smaller  motor  root  taking  up  a  position  median  to 
the  larger  sensory  root. 

Here  the  motor  root  comes  into  connection  with  a  collection  of 
nerve  cells  (Figs.  115  and  112,  V.  m.),  which  may  be  regarded 
as  its  nucleus;  but  this  is  not  the  whole  nucleus  of  the  motor 
root.  From  the  level  of  the  nucleus  there  stretches  forwards  as 
far  as  the  level  of  the  anterior  corpora  quadrigemina  a  bundle  of 
longitudinal  fibres  which,  since  it  is  usually  traced  from  the  front 
backwards  until  it  passes  into  the  root  of  the  nerve,  is  spoken  of 
as  the  descending  root  of  the  fifth  nerve. 

This  descending  root  begins  as  a  few  scattered  bundles  of  fibres 
at  the  level  of  the  anterior  corpora  quadrigemina,  in  the  peri- 
pheral lateral  part  of  the  central  grey  matter  surrounding  the 
aqueduct,  dorsal  and  lateral  (Fig.  1 14,  V.  d),  to  the  nucleus  of  the 
third  nerve  (Fig.  114,  III.  ??.).  From  thence  the  fibres  pass 
backward,  augmenting  in  number,  and  soon  form  a  compact 
bundle,  semilunar  in  transverse  section,  lying  lateral  to  the  fourth 
nerve  as  this  is  rising  dorsally  (Fig.  113,  V.  d.) ;  still  increasing 
in  number  in  their  course  backward  they  gradually  assume  a 
more  ventral  position  as  the  aqueduct  opens  into  the  fourth 
ventricle.  All  along  its  course  this  descending  root  has  attached 
to  it  large  (70  /j,  or  more  in  diameter),  sparse  spheroidal  nerve 


CHAP.  IL] 


THE   BRAIN. 


963 


cells,  of  striking  appearance  ;  these  however  seem  too  few  to  give 
origin  to  at  least  all  the  fibres,  and  there  are  some  reasons  for 
connecting  this  root  with  the  collection  of  grey  matter  called 
'  locus  caeruleus'.  Fig.  113,  I.e. 

We  may  probably  regard  this  descending  root  as  belonging  to 
the  motor  division  of  the  nerve  ;  but  it  is  stated  that  many  of 
the  fibres  of  this  root  pass  into  the  sensory  root,  eventually 
finding  their  way,  according  to  some  observers,  into  the  ophthalmic 
branch. 

The  sensory  root  may  be  similarly  traced  into  a  nucleus,  the 
sensory  nucleus  (Figs.  115  and  112,  V.  s.)  lying  lateral  to  the 
motor  nucleus,  and  connected  with  this  is  the  striking  tract  of 
fibres,  to' which  already  we  have  so  frequently  alluded,  and  which 
is  called  the  ascending  root  of  the  fifth  nerve. 


-Yd. 


FIG.  113.     THBOUGH  THE  FORE  PART  OF  THE  PONS.     (Sherrington.) 
(In  the  line  113,  Fig.  108.) 

Py.  Pyramidal  fibres.  F.  C.  Fibres  from  the  frontal  cortex.  S.  P.  Superior  Peduncle 
of  the  cerebellum.  F  m.  median  portion,  F  I.  lateral  portion  of  the  Fillet. 
1.  posterior  longitudinal  bundles.  P.  C.  Q.  Posterior  corpora  quadrigemina. 
y.  Fibres  which  become  detached  from  the  Fillet,  and  further  forward  form 
(the  innermost)  part  of  the  Pes  of  the  Crus.  I.  c.  locus  caeruleus.  n.  P.  Q. 
nucleus  of  the  posterior  corpora  quadrigemina ;  the  outline  is  made  too  sharp. 
IV.  bundles  of  the  fourth  nerve  decussating,  IV.  n.  its  nucleus.  V.  d.  descend- 
ing root  of  the  fifth  nerve.  Aq.  the  aqueduct,  c.  g.  the  region  of  central  grey 
matter. 

This  ascending  root  begins  as  a  bundle  or  bundles  of  few  fibres 
which  may  be  traced  backward  as  far  as  at  least  the  level  of  the 
second  cervical  nerve,  and  is  soon  conspicuous  in  transverse  sections 
(Fig.  109  et  seq.t  V.  a.)  as  a  semilunar  patch  of  white  matter  forming 
a  sort  of  cap  on  the  outside  of  the  swollen  caput  of  the  posterior 


964 


NUCLEI   OF   CRANIAL  NERVES.          [BOOK  m. 


horn,  between  this  structure  and  the  longitudinal  fibres  which  are 
beginning  to  form  the  restiform  body  on  the  surface.  Passing 
upwards,  and  continually  augmenting  in  bulk,  the  root  clings,  as 
it  were,  to  the  gelatinous  substance  of  the  caput  of  the  posterior 
horn,  and  sinks  with  it  inwardly  and  ventrally  as  this  becomes 
covered  up  first  by  the  restiform  body  and  subsequently  by  the 
issuing  trunk  of  the  great  eighth  or  auditory  nerve  (Figs.  110, 
111).  Passing  still  forward,  beyond  the  disappearing  gelatinous 
substance,  the  root,  still  growing  larger  and  divided  into  several 
distinct  bundles,  runs  into  the  reticular  formation  of  the  pons 
and,  reaching  the  level  of  the  sensory  nucleus,  suddenly  bends 
round  and  joins  the  sensory  root. 

This  ascending  root  differs   from  the  descending  root  in  not 


FIG.  114.    THROUGH  THE  CRUS  AND  ANTERIOR  CORPORA  QUADRIGEMINA. 
(One  half  only  is  shewn.)     (Sherrington.) 

(In  the  line  114,  Fig.  108.) 

Py.  the  pyramidal  portion  of  the  pes.  Fr.  the  region  of  the  pes  occupied  by  fibres 
from  the  frontal  portion  of  the  cortex.  Pr.  O.  the  region  occupied  by  fibres 
coming  from  the  occipital  portion  of  the  cortex,  y.  fibres  coming  from  the 
fillet.  Op.  the  optic  tract.  F.  the  fillet,  I.  the  lateral  portion,  m.  the  median 
portion.  I.  the  posterior  longitudinal  bundle.  B.  a.  the  brachium  of  the 
anterior  corpus  quadrigeminum.  x.  fibres  from  the  posterior  commissure  of 
the  cerebrum,  r.  raphe.  S.  n.  substantia  nigra.  R.  n.  red  nucleus.  C.  g.  I. 
lateral,  and  C.  g.  m.  median  corpus  geniculatum.  Pvr.  pulvinar  of  optic 
thalamus.  A.  Q.  n.  nucleus  or  grey  matter  of  anterior  corpus  quadrigeminum. 
III.  n.  nucleus  of  III.  third  nerve.  III'.  Eootlets  from  the  dorsal  part  of 
III.  n.  the  nucleus  of  the  third  nerve  which  cross  the  median  line  to  emerge 
with  rootlets  derived  from  the  nucleus  of  the  opposite  side.  s.  m.  superficial 
layer  of  fibres  of  the  ant.  corp.  quad.  d.  m.  deep  layer.  V.  d.  descending  root 
of  the  fifth  nerve.  Aq.  aqueduct  surrounded  by  cerebral  grey  matter. 

having  conspicuously  attached  to  it  any  collection  of  nerve  cells ; 
in  this  respect  it  resembles  the  ascending  root  of  the  glosso- 
pharyngeal,  and  we  may  add  part  of  the  posterior  root  of  an 


CHAP,  ii.]  THE   BRAIN.  965 

ordinary  spinal  nerve,  the  fibres  of  which,  as  we  have  seen,  pass 
into  the  grey  matter  without  being  obviously  connected  with 
nerve  cells.  In  its  lower  part  at  least  it  consists  of  extremely  fine 
fibres,  and  indeed  looks  very  much  like  a  continuation  in  the  bulb 
of  the  marginal  (Lissauer's)  zone  of  the  spinal  cord. 

§  622.     The  Fourth  or  Trochlear  Nerve.    The  nucleus  of  this 
nerve  (Fig.  115,  IV.)  is  a  column  of  somewhat  large  multipolar  ^ 
cells  on  each  side  of  the  median  line  below  the  aqueduct  (Fig.  ' 
113,   IV.    7i.),  reaching   from   the   level   of  the  junction   of  the 
anterior  and  posterior  corpora  quadrigemina  to  the  hinder  level 
of  the  latter  body. 

The  root,  starting  from  the  lateral  surface  of  the  nucleus,  does 
not  take  at  first  a  ventral  direction,  but  sweeps  laterally  and 
dorsally  in  the  outer  layers  of  the  central  grey  matter  (Fig.  113), 
and  so  curving  round  to  the  dorsal  surface  reaches  the  valve  of 
Vieussens,  where  in  the  median  line  it  decussates  with  its  fellow 
in  the  substance  of  the  valve;  such  a  decussation  at  a  distance 
from  the  nucleus  of  origin  is  exceptional  in  the  cranial  nerves. 
Leaving  the  surface  of  the  brain  in  the  valve,  it  takes  a  superficial 
course  curving  (Fig.  108,  B)  laterally  and  ventrally,  and  makes 
its  appearance  in  a  ventral  view  of  the  brain  at  the  front  edge  of 
the  pons,  on  the  lateral  edge  of  the  crus  (Fig.  108,  (7.). 

§  623.  The  Third  or  Oculomotor  Nerve.  The  nucleus  of  this 
nerve  (Fig.  115,  III.,  114,  III.  n.)  is  a  column  of,  for  the  most  part, 
fairly  large  multipolar  cells  lying  on  each  side  close  to  the  median 
line,  in  the  grey  matter  of  the  central  canal,  just  dorsal  to  a 
bundle  of  fibres  which  we  shall  speak  of  as  the  longitudinal 
posterior  bundle;  it  reaches  from  the  level  of  the  posterior 
commissure  in  the  third  ventricle  to  the  level  of  the  junction 
of  the  anterior  and  posterior  corpora  quadrigemina.  In  a  section 
taken  through  its  middle  (Fig.  114)  the  nucleus  is  seen  to  give 
off  fibres  which  run  vertically  towards  the  ventral  surface, 
traversing  the  tegmentum  and  a  body  (Rn.)  which  we  shall 
presently  speak  of  as  the  "  red  nucleus,"  but  apparently  making 
no  connections  with  these  structures,  and  pierce  the  median  edge 
of  the  pes,  emerging  (Fig.  108,  (7.)  on  the  surface  to  the  median 
side  of  each  crus.  As  we  shall  see  later  on,  this  nerve  is  now 
exclusively  efferent,  whatever  it  may  have  been  in  more  primitive 
beings.  We  shall  also  see  later  on,  that  impulses  starting  from 
the  cerebrum  of  one  side  pass  to  the  nerve  of  the  other  side,  that 
is  to  say  decussate  ;  and  this  is  also  the  case  with  the  other 
efferent  cranial  nerves.  The  fibres  which  appear  to  take  origin 
from  the  nerve  cells  of  the  nucleus  do  not  cross  over  after 
emerging  from  the  nucleus,  but  keep  to  the  same  side;  there 
is  no  distant  decussation  as  in  the  case  just  noted  of  the  fourth 
nerve.  There  are  however  fibres  (Fig.  114,  III.')  which  leaving 
the  nucleus  cross  the  median  raphe  from  one  side  to  the  other, 
and  these  possibly  are  the  paths  for  the  decussation  of  the 


966 


NUCLEI   OF   CRANIAL   NERVES.         [BOOK  in. 


CHAP.  IL]  THE   BRAIN.  967 

FIG.  115.     DIAGRAM  TO  ILLUSTRATE  THE  POSITION  OP  THE  NUCLEI  OF  THE  CRANIAL 
NERVES.     (Sherrington.) 

The  brain  is  supposed  to  be  viewed  from  the  dorsal  aspect,  the  cerebral  hemispheres 
and  cerebellum  having  been  cut  away.  The  nuclei  are  represented  as  if  seen 
through  transparent  material.  On  the  right  side,  the  corpus  striatum  and 
optic  thalamus  have  been  cut  away  horizontally  to  some  little  depth  in  order 
to  shew  their  internal  structure. 

L.  lateral,  E.  P.  external  posterior  and  M.  P.  median  posterior  column  of  the 
cord.  I.  P.  inferior  peduncle,  S.  P.  superior  peduncle,  and  P.  middle  peduncle 
of  the  cerebellum,  all  cut  across.  The  dotted  curved  lines,  upper  and  lower,  on 
the  right  half  of  the  figure  to  which  the  dotted  line  P.  V.  outside  the  figure 
points,  mark  the  upper  and  lower  boundaries  of  the  pons  on  the  ventral  aspect. 

The  outline  of  the  fourth  ventricle  is  shewn  by  a  bold  thick  line.  In  the  floor  of 
the  ventricle  are  shewn,  on  the  right  half: — fp.  fovea  posterior.  Th.  trigonum 
hypoglossi.  T.  ac.  trigonum  acusticum.  e.  t.  eminentia  teres.  s.  in.  striae 
medullares  or  acusticae.  /.  a.  fovea  anterior.  I.  c.  locus  caeruleus.  I.  g.  valve 
of  Vieussens. 

Qp.  posterior  and  Qa.  anterior  corpus  quadrigeminum.  Pg.  pineal  gland.  Nr. 
the  outline  of  the  red  nucleus.  3,  the  third  ventricle,  in  which  C  indicates 
the  middle  or  soft  commissure.  F.  p.  a.  the  pillars  of  the  fornix,  behind 
which  is  indicated  in  the  cavity  of  the  third  ventricle  the  hollow  of  the 
infundibulum.  C.  C.  g.  the  genu  of  the  corpus  callosum,  between  which  and 
the  fornix  the  cavity  often  called  the  fifth  ventricle  is  indicated.  F.  portion 
of  convolution  of  frontal  hemisphere  cut  across. 

On  the  left  side  are  shewn : — C.  S.  corpus  striatum.  0.  T.  optic  thalamus.  Pv. 
pulvinar.  T.  a.  Tuberculum  anterius.  ch.  s.  choroidal  sulcus  marking  the 
place  of  reflection  of  the  choroidal  plexus.  On  the  right  side  are  exposed : — 
NC.  head  of,  Nc,  end  of  tail  of  nucleus  caudatus.  dp',  dp"  the  two  parts 
of  the  globus  pallidus,  and  Pt,  putamen  of  the  nucleus  lenticularis.  N.  a. 
anterior  nucleus.  N.  med.  median  nucleus,  N.  lat.  lateral  nucleus  and  Pv'. 
pulvinar  of  the  optic  thalamus.  Cla.  front  limb,  Gig.  knee  or  genu,  Cip.  hind 
limb  of  internal  capsule.  Ce.  external  capsule.  Cl.  claustrum. 

The  numerals  III.  to  XII.  indicate  the  nuclei  of  the  respective  cranial  nerves,  all 
shewn  on  the  left  side  with  the  exception  of  the  accessory-vago-glosso- 
pharyngeal  IX.  X.  XI.,  which  to  avoid  confusion  is  placed  on  the  right  side. 
V.  is  the  motor  nucleus  of  the  fifth  nerve  with  the  descending  root,  V.  a.  the 
sensory  nucleus  of  the  same  with  the  long  ascending  root.  VIII.  m.  median 
nucleus,  VIII.  I.  lateral  nucleus  of  the  auditory  nerve,  n.  a.  nucleus  ambiguus. 
The  ascending  root  of  the  ninth  nerve  is  seen  at  the  hind  end  of  the  combined 
nucleus  of  IX.  X.  XI. 

impulses;  but  they  may  be  fibres  passing  from  the  crus  across 
the  raphe  to  the  nucleus.  This  nerve  has  special  relations  with 
the  optic  tract,  but  of  these  we  shall  speak  when  we  come  to 
deal  with  the  functions  of  the  nerves. 

§  624.  In  attempting  to  understand  the  nature  and  relations 
of  these  cranial  nerves,  it  must  be  borne  in  mind  that,  while 
morphological  studies  lead  us  to  believe  that,  as  the  vertebrate 
body  has  been  developed  out  of  an  invertebrate  ancestry,  so  the 
brain  of  the  vertebrate  has  arisen  by  a  series  of  modifications 
from  the  nervous  structures  placed  at  the  head  and  around  the 
mouth  of  an  invertebrate,  the  same  studies  teach  us  that  such 
an  evolution  has  been  accomplished  by  means  of  profound 
changes.  We  have,  for  instance,  reason  to  think  that  the 
mouth  of  the  vertebrate  does  not  correspond  to  the  mouth  of 
the  invertebrate,  but  is  a  new  structure,  whose  appearance  has 


968  NUCLEI   OF   CRANIAL   NERVES.         [BOOK  HI. 

been  accompanied  by  a  considerable  dislocation  of  parts.  We 
must  accordingly  expect  to  find  the  indications  of  a  segmental 
arrangement  greatly  obscured  on  the  one  hand  by  transposition, 
and  on  the  other  by  fusion. 

The  twelfth  or  hypoglossal  nerve  is  one  whose  nature  seems 
fairly  simple.  It  is  in  function  exclusively  an  efferent  nerve.  The 
large  cells,  with  conspicuous  axis- cylinder  processes,  which  charac- 
terize its  nucleus,  are  exactly  like  those  of  the  anterior  horn  of 
the  spinal  cord  which  give  origin  to  the  fibres  of  an  anterior  root. 
The  nucleus  moreover  in  its  position  corresponds  to  part  of  the 
anterior  horn  of  the  spinal  cord,  if  we  take  into  account  the 
shifting  involved  in  the  decussation  of  the  pyramids,  and  in  the 
new  developments  of  the  bulb.  If  we  compare  Fig.  109  with  any 
section  of  the  cord,  we  see  that  the  hypoglossal  nerve  corresponds 
to  an  anterior  root  of  the  spinal  cord,  but  that  the  fibres,  after 
leaving  the  cells  from  which  they  take  origin,  traverse  in  the 
former  a  large  tract,  and  in  the  latter  case  a  small  tract  of  tissue. 
Whether  the  whole  nerve  corresponds  to  the  fibres  of  several 
segments  fused  together,  or  to  those  of  one  segment  spread  out 
longitudinally,  is  for  our  present  purposes  of  secondary  importance. 

Recognizing  the  hypoglossal  nerve  as  the  homologue  of  a 
spinal  anterior  root,  we  may  go  on  to  claim  the  nuclei  of  the  third 
and  fourth  nerves  as  similar  groups  of  cells  of  the  anterior  horn, 
giving  rise  to  anterior  roots.  The  position  of  the  nuclei,  the 
character  of  the  cells,  the  function  of  the  fibres,  all  support  this 
view.  The  case  is  perhaps  not  so  clear  as  that  of  the  hypoglossal 
nerve,  since  there  are  reasons  for  thinking  that  these  nerves  have 
undergone  in  the  course  of  evolution  greater  changes  than  has  the 
hypoglossal  nerve ;  still  these  reasons  do  not  oppose  the  above 
conclusion. 

The  nucleus  of  the  exclusively  motor  sixth  nerve  does  not 
exactly  correspond  to  those  of  the  third  and  fourth  in  position; 
but  we  may  probably  place  it  in  the  same  series  with  them. 
Thus  we  have  in  succession,  the  third,  fourth,  sixth,  and  twelfth 
nerves,  with  their  respective  nuclei,  as  the  anterior  roots  of  nerves 
of  their  several  segments. 

In  the  fifth  nerve,  the  dislocation  and  fusion  spoken  of  above 
has  introduced  difficulties.  The  motor  nucleus,  with  the  fibres  of 
the  motor  root  to  which  it  gives  origin,  has  by  some  been  con- 
sidered as  homologous  to  the  series  just  described ;  but  it  is  at 
once  obvious  that  we  cannot  look  upon  this  great  fifth  nerve  as 
corresponding  to  one  spinal  nerve,  with  its  anterior  and  posterior 
root,  great  as  the  superficial  resemblance  seems  to  be.  The  features 
of  the  remarkable  ascending  root  forbid  this.  The  fibres  of  this 
root  may  be  traced  back,  as  we  have  said,  to  the  very  beginning  of 
the  bulb,  and  indeed  into  the  spinal  cord  beyond ;  as  far  as  can 
be-  ascertained,  they  are  not  in  an  obvious  and  direct  manner 
connected  with  nerve  cells  along  their  course ;  but  the  bundle  of 


CHAP,  ii.]  THE   BRAIN.  969 

fibres  clings,  as  we  have  seen,  to  the  gelatinous  substance  of  the 
posterior  horn  of  the  spinal  cord  and  to  the  continuation  of  this 
along  the  bulb,  and  the  fibres  are  lost  in  this  structure.  The 
root,  therefore,  as  we  have  said,  corresponds  very  closely  to  part 
at  least  of  the  posterior  root  of  a  spinal  nerve,  and,  though  the 
matter  has  not  yet  been  experimentally  proved,  we  may  infer 
that  the  trophic  centres  of  these  fibres  are  to  be  found  in  the 
cells  of  the  Gasserian  ganglion. 

But  if  this  ascending  root  be  of  the  nature  of  a  posterior  root 
(and  we  may  incidentally  remark  that  the  term  ascending  has 
been  unhappily  chosen,  since,  if  it  be  an  afferent  root,  the  direction 
of  the  impulses  which  it  carries  will  be  a  descending  one,  namely 
from  the  entrance  in  the  pons  towards  the  hinder  parts),  we  can 
hardly  suppose  that  it  belongs  to  a  single  segment,  or  is  the  com- 
plement of  the  motor  root  alone  ;  in  it,  most  probably,  the  posterior 
fibres  of  several  segments  are  blended  together.  Further,  we  may 
perhaps  infer  that  the  other  fibres  of  the  sensory  root  which  end 
directly  in  what  we  have  called  the  sensory  nucleus,  are  in  nature 
quite  distinct  from  the  fibres  of  the  ascending  root ;  and  if  so, 
difficulties  arise  as  to  the  nature  and  homologies  of  the  nucleus 
in  question.  These,  however,  we  must  not  discuss  here,  nor  can 
we  enter  into  the  question  of  the  nature  of  the  descending  root, 
concerning  the  fibres  of  which,  as  we  have  said,  authorities  differ 
as  to  whether  they  pass  into  the  motor  or  sensory  root.  We  have 
said  enough  to  shew  that  this  fifth  nerve  is  extremely  complex, 
and  that  its  apparent  conformity  to  a  simple  spinal  nerve  is  in 
reality  misleading.  » 

The  fibres  of  the  vagus,  glossopharyngeal,  and  bulbar  accessory, 
taken  together,  are  partly  efferent,  partly  afferent.  The  combined 
nucleus  of  these  three  nerves,  the  cells  of  which  are  small  and 
devoid  of  conspicuous  axis-cylinder  processes,  is  usually  regarded 
as  a  sensory  nucleus,  and  in  the  diagram,  Fig.  115,  is  shaded 
accordingly.  It  may  perhaps  be  compared  to  the  sensory  nucleus 
of  the  fifth.  Thus,  the  ascending  root,  or  fasciculus  solitarius, 
presents  many  analogies  with  the  ascending  root  of  the  fifth,  and 
we  are  led  to  regard  this  as,  like  it,  a  gathering  of  certain  afferent 
fibres  of  the  posterior  roots  of  several  segments ;  in  its  case  also 
the  term  ascending  is  misleading.  But  .there  are  many  difficulties 
in  connection  with  this  nucleus,  as  with  the  fifth.  We  must  not 
enter  into  a  detailed  discussion  concerning  them,  but  may  remark 
that  we  have  here  perhaps  to  deal  with  complexities  due  to  the 
fact  that  certainly  many  vagus  and  glossopharyngeal  fibres,  and 
probably  some  of  those  of  the  fifth,  are  splanchnic  in  function. 

The  nucleus  ambiguus  contains  large  conspicuous  cells  and  we 
may  probably  regard  it  as  a  motor  nucleus,  especially  of  the  vagus 
fibres.  We  may  also  perhaps  place  it  and  the  nucleus  of  the 
seventh  nerve  in  the  same  category,  and  further  class  with  them 
the  motor  nucleus  of  the  fifth,  looking  upon  all  three  as  so 


970  INTERMEDIATE   GREY   MATTER.         [BOOK  in. 

many  detached  portions  of  grey  matter,  corresponding  to  some 
part  of  the  anterior  horn  of  the  spinal  cord.  Whether  they  are 
exactly  homologous  to  the  hypoglossal  nucleus,  and  their  fibres 
to  simple  anterior  roots,  is  not  so  clear. 

Lastly,  the  auditory  nerve,  both  from  its  character  as  a  nerve 
of  special  sense  and  from  the  remarkable  features  of  its  nuclei,  is 
even  more  difficult.  Most  probably  it  results  from  the  fusion  of 
more  roots  than  one ;  but  it  is  impossible  at  present  to  obtain  a 
clear  conception  of  the  nature  of  the  whole  nerve. 


2.     The  Superficial  Grey  Matter. 

§  625.  The  whole  of  the  surface  of  each  cerebral  hemisphere 
for  some  little  depth  inwards  consists  of  grey  matter,  possessing 
special  characters ;  this  is  called  the  cortical  grey  matter,  or  the 
cortex  cerebri,  or  shortly  and  simply  the  cortex.  As  we  shall  see, 
by  its  histological  and  still  more  by  its  physiological  features,  it 
stands  apart  from  all  other  kinds  of  grey  matter. 

The  whole  of  the  surface  of  the  cerebellum  is  also  covered  with 
grey  matter,  which,  while  possessing  features  of  its  own,  so  far 
resembles  the  cerebral  cortex  in  its  histological  characters  that 
it  too  has  been  spoken  of  as  cortex,  as  the  cortex  cerebelli.  By 
its  functional  manifestations,  however,  it  differs  widely  from  the 
cerebral  cortex ;  and  since  there  are  many  advantages  in  being 
able  to  use  the  word  cortex  in  connection  with  the  cerebrum 
only,  it  is  desirable  not  to  speak  of  a  cerebellar  cortex  but  to 
employ  the  term  "  superficial  grey  matter  of  the  cerebellum." 

The  third  ventricle  and  the  hinder  part  of  the  fourth  ventricle 
are  not  roofed  in  by  nervous  material,  and  possess  no  superficial 
grey  matter  at  all.  In  the  corpora  quadrigemina,  which  form  the 
roof  of  the  aqueduct  or  cavity  of  the  mid-brain,  grey  matter  is 
present  and  possesses,  in  the  case  of  the  anterior  corpora  quadri- 
gemina at  least,  characters  to  a  certain  extent  analogous  to  those 
of  the  cortex  and  to  the  cerebellar  superficial  grey  matter ;  but  it 
will  be  best  to  consider  the  grey  matter  of  these  bodies  as 
belonging  to  another  category. 

3.     The  Intermediate  Grey  Matter  of  the  Crural 
System. 

§  626.  We  have  seen  (§  603)  that  the  crura  cerebri  form  the 
prominent  part  of  a  system  of  longitudinal  fibres  stretching  from 
each  cerebral  hemisphere  to  the  bulb  and  to  the  spinal  cord. 
This  system  of  fibres,  upon  which  we  may  consider  the  various 
parts  of  the  brain  to  be  as  it  were  founded,  we  may  speak  of 
as  the  crural  system.  It  is,  it  is  true,  not  one  continuous  strand, 
but  a  number  of  different  strands,  having  different  beginnings 


CHAP,  ii.]  THE   BRAIN.  971 

and  endings;  but  these  all  contribute  to  the  crura  and  are 
so  far  alike  as  to  justify  us  in  considering  them  as  a  system. 
The  cortical  grey  matter  of  each  hemisphere  is,  as  we  shall  see, 
connected  with  various  parts  of  this  system,  and  in  one  sense 
we  may  regard  this  system  as  beginning  in  the  cortex  of  each 
hemisphere,  and  ending  in  the  spinal  cord.  But  certain  masses 
of  grey  matter  in  the  hemisphere  not  strictly  cortical,  and  several 
important  masses  and  areas  of  grey  matter  lying  between  the 
hemisphere  and  the  cord,  are  connected  with  the  system ;  and 
these  we  may  speak  of  as  the  "  intermediate  grey  matter  of  the 
crural  system." 

Corpus  striatum  and  optic  thalamus.  Of  all  these  several 
collections  of  grey  matter,  the  largest,  most  conspicuous,  and 
perhaps  the  most  important  are  the  two  masses  in  the  front 
part  of  the  system  known  as  the  corpus  striatum  and  optic 
thalamus.  The  former  is,  as  we  have  seen  (§  602),  a  development 
of  the  wall  of  the  cerebral  vesicle,  the  latter  a  development  of 
the  wall  of  the  vesicle  of  the  third  ventricle.  They  are  therefore 
of  different  origin ;  although  in  the  course  of  the  growth  of  the 
brain  they  become  closely  attached  to  each  other,  they  are  at  the 
outset  quite  separate  and  distinct.  Moreover,  as  we  shall  see, 
they  differ  from  each  other  so  essentially,  in  their  nature  and 
relations,  that  they  cannot  be  considered  as  homologous  bodies ; 
and  the  term  "  basal  ganglia  "  often  applied  to  them  is  therefore 
unfortunate.  Nevertheless  it  will  render  the  description  of  their 
topographical  relations  easier,  if  for  a  little  while  we  consider 
them  together. 

When  the  lateral  ventricle  is  laid  open  from  above,  part  of  the 
corpus  striatum  is  seen  projecting  into  the  cavity  of  the  ventricle. 
In  front  the  projecting  part  is  broad,  forming  the  lateral  wall  and 
part  of  the  floor  of  the  ventricle,  and  to  its  median  side  lies  the 
cavity  of  the  ventricle,  separated  from  its  fellow  by  the  septum 
lucidum.  Farther  back  the  projecting  part,  becoming  gradually 
narrower,  assumes  a  more  lateral  position  and  passes  into  the 
descending  horn.  In  this  part  of  its  course  there  lies  on  its 
median  side,  separated  from  it  by  a  narrow  band  called  the 
tsenia  semicircularis  or  stria  terminalis,  the  optic  thalamus,  a 
narrow7  strip  of  the  surface  of  which  is  seen  projecting  outside 
the  edge  of  the  choroid  plexus.  If  now,  not  only  both  lateral 
ventricles  be  laid  open  by  removal  of  the  corpus  callosum  and 
the  fornix  with  the  velum  interpositum  and  choroid  plexus  be 
taken  away,  so  as  fully  to  expose  the  third  ventricle,  but  also, 
in  order  to  obtain  a  better  view,  the  whole  of  the  hinder  part 
of  the  cerebrum  containing  the  posterior  horns  of  the  lateral 
ventricle,  be  completely  cut  away,  it  is  seen  (Fig.  115)  that  the 
two  optic  thalami  (0.  T.)  present  themselves  as  two  large  oval 
bodies,  placed  obliquely  athwart  the  diverging  crura  cerebri  and 
converging  in  front  to  form  the  immediate  walls  of  the  third 


972  INTERNAL   CAPSULE.  [BOOK  HI. 

ventricle.  In  front  and  to  the  sides  of  the  optic  thalami  are  seen 
the  corpora  striata  (C.  8.)  forming  anteriorly  the  lateral  walls  of 
the  two  lateral  ventricles,  and  diverging  behind  to  allow  of  the 
interposition  of  the  optic  thalami.  On  each  side  of  the  brain 
then  these  two  bodies,  the  corpus  striatum  and  optic  thalamus, 
appear  as  two  masses  of  grey  matter  placed  on  the  crus  cerebri 
as  this,  diverging  from  its  fellow,  begins  to  spread  out  into  the 
cerebral  hemisphere,  the  corpus  striatum  being  placed  somewhat 
in  front  of  the  optic  thalamus.  The  relations  of  the  two  bodies 
moreover  are  such  that  while  the  optic  thalamus  alone  forms  the 
wall  of  the  third  ventricle  to  which  it  properly  belongs,  and  the 
corpus  striatum  forms  part  of  the  wall  of  the  lateral  ventricle  to 
which  it  in  turn  properly  belongs,  the  optic  thalamus  also  projects 
into  and  seems  to  form  part  of  the  wall  of  the  lateral  ventricle, 
though  at  its  origin  it  had  nothing  to  do  with  the  cerebral  vesicle. 

We  spoke  just  now  of  these  bodies  as  being  placed  on  the 
crura  cerebri,  but  though  their  dorsal  surfaces  thus  project  from 
the  dorsal  surface  of  the  diverging  crura,  a  large  portion  of  each 
body  is,  so  to  speak,  imbedded  in  the  substance  of  the  diverging 
crus,  and  what  is  seen  in  the  above  surface  view  is  only  a  part 
of  each  body,  and  indeed,  in  the  case  of  the  corpus  striatum,  only 
a,  small  part.  In  order  to  understand  the  nature  and  relations  of 
these  two  important  bodies  we  must  study  sections  taken  through 
a  cerebral  hemisphere  in  various  planes  (Figs.  116 — 123). 

Each  crus  is  made  up  as  we  have  seen  of  a  dorsal  portion  or 
tegmentum  consisting  largely  of  grey  matter,  and  a  ventral  portion 
or  pes  consisting  exclusively  of  longitudinally  disposed  fibres. 
The  tegmentum  ends  partly  in  structures  lying  ventral  to  the 
thalamus,  partly  in  the  thalamus  itself;  and  we  may  for  the 
present  leave  this  part  of  the  crus  out  of  consideration.  The 
fibres  of  the  pes,  while  continuing  their  oblique  course  forwards 
and  outwards,  soon  rise  dorsally  by  the  side  of  the  thalamus  and 
hence,  in  a  transverse  dorso-veutral  section  at  the  level  of  the  hind 
part  of  the  thalamus  (Fig.  116),  are  seen  leaving  their  previous 
position  ventral  to  the  substantia  nigra  (Sn)  and  passing  (dp)  by 
the  side  of  the  thalamus  on  their  way  to  the  central  white  matter 
of  the  hemisphere.  In  this  part  of  their  course  they  form  a  thick 
strand  separating  the  thalamus  (In)  from  a  large  mass  of  grey 
matter  which,  roughly  triangular  in  section,  is  divided  by  parti- 
tions of  white  matter  into  three  parts  (Gp ',  Gp",  Pt\  and  of  which 
we  shall  speak  directly  as  the  nucleus  lenticularis. 

If  instead  of  taking  a  transverse  we  take  a  longitudinal  dorso- 
ventral  (or  as  it  is  called  sagittal)  section  (Fig.  122)  we  find  that 
the  fibres  forming  the  strand  in  question  do  not  continue  parallel 
to  each  other  as  they  rise  dorsally  but  diverge  in  a  radiating 
manner,  forming  the  so-called  corona  radiata.  If  again  we  take 
horizontal  sections  at  proper  levels  (Figs.  115,  121),  we  find  that 
this  strand  or  rather  thick  band  of  dorsally  directed  radiating 


CHAP,  ii.] 


THE   BRAIN. 


973 


FlG.  116.      DIAGRAMMATIC  OUTLINE  OF  A  TRANSVEBSE   DORSOVENTRAL  SECTION  THROUGH 

THE  EIGHT   HEMISPHERE  (MAN),   AT   LEVEL  POSTERIOR  TO   THE   KNEE  OF  THE 
INTERNAL  CAPSULE.     (Natural  size.)     (Sherrington.) 

Nc,  nucleus  caudatus;  in  the  upper  part  of  the  figure,  the  section  of  the  nucleus 
is  through  the  narrower  portion  which  succeeds  the  wider  front  end  or  head; 
in  the  lower  part  of  the  figure  the  section  passes  through  the  tail  of  the 
nucleus  near  its  end,  and  this  portion  of  it  has  for  the  sake  of  clearness  been 
sundered  from  the  grey  matter  of  Na,  nucleus  amygdalae,  more  distinctly  than 
in  reality  is  the  case.  Gp',  Gp"  globus  pallidus,  seen  here  in  two  segments,  and 
Pt.  putamen  of  nucleus  lenticularis.  an.  the  anterior,  in.  the  inner,  and  In.  the 
lateral  nucleus  of  the  optic  thalamus;  at  II.  is  seen  the  "latticed  layer"  lying 
next  to  Cip.  the  posterior  limb  of  the  internal  capsule  and  containing  many 
strands  of  fibres  which  mingle  with  it.  In  the  thalamus  between  the  anterior 
and  internal  nuclei  on  the  one  hand  and  the  lateral  nucleus  on  the  other  is  a 
layer  shaded  less  deeply  in  the  figure,  representing  the  internal  medullary  lamina 
of  the  thalamus,  consisting  largely  of  white  matter.  Other  collections  of  white 
matter  within  the  thalamus  are  Vb,  the  bundle  of  Vicq.  d' Azyr  and  F'  the  lower 
end  of  the  anterior  pillar  of  the  fornix.  F.  The  upper  end  of  the  anterior 
pillar  of  the  fornix,  below  cc  the  corpus  callosum.  Csb.  corpus  subthalamicum, 
forming  a  fairly  continuous  mass  with  the  thalamus ;  Sn.  substantia  nigra. 
cl.  Claustrum ;  ce.  external  capsule.  Ca.  terminal  portion  of  anterior  com- 


974  INTERNAL   CAPSULE.  [BOOK  HI. 

missure.  In.  the  insula  or  island  of  Keil.  Iv.  the  lateral  ventricle;  L  v.  d. 
descending  horn  of  lateral  ventricle;  V.  3.  in  the  position  of  the  third 
ventricle ;  the  outlines  of  the  cavities  are  made  diagrammatically  distinct  by 
thick  black  lines.  Op.  optic  tract;  P,  P.  Parietal  lobe.  T.  Temporal  lobe. 

fibres  not  only  stretches  (dp)  between  the  thalamus  and  the  grey 
mass  just  spoken  of,  but  reaching  farther  forward  passes  (Gia) 
between  the  same  grey  mass  on  the  lateral  side  and  another  grey 
mass  (Nc)  on  the  median  side,  the  latter  from  its  position  being 
evidently  the  part  of  the  corpus  striatum  which  projects  into  the 
lateral  ventricle.  The  same  horizontal  sections  further  teach  us 
that  the  front  part  of  the  band  (Cia)  is  bent  at  an  angle  upon  the 
hind  part  (Oip). 

It  appears  then  from  these  sections  that  the  fibres  of  the  pes 
as  they  rise  up  dorsally  into  the  hemisphere  spread  out  in  the 
form  of  a  fan  bent  upon  itself.  This  fan-like  expansion  of  the  pes 
is  called  the  internal  capsule,  the  angle  formed  by  the  bend  being 
called  its  genu  or  knee,  (Gig}  the  part  in  front  of  the  knee  the 
front  limb,  and  the  part  behind  the  knee  the  hind  limb.  And 
horizontal  sections  at  levels  more  dorsal  than  those  given  in 
Figs.  115 — 121  would  shew  that  the  fibres  composing  this  fan- 
like  internal  capsule,  as  they  rose  dorsally,  curved  away  in  various 
directions  to  reach  nearly  all  parts  of  the  surface  of  the  hemisphere. 
We  may  add  that  though  the  internal  capsule  is  mainly  composed 
of  fibres  which  thus  stretch  all  the  way  from  the  cerebral  cortex 
to  the  pes  of  the  crus,  it  also  contains  other  fibres  of  which  we 
shall  speak  later  on. 

§  627.  The  grey  mass  separated  from  the  thalamus  by  the 
hind  limb  of  the  internal  capsule  is  called  as  a  whole  the  nucleus 
lenticularis,  since  in  horizontal  section  it  presents  a  certain  though 
distant  resemblance  to  a  lens.  Of  the  three  divisions  into  which 
it  is  split  up  by  the  partitions  of  white  matter,  the  two  median 
ones  Up',  Gp"  are  spoken  of  together  as  the  globus  pallidus,  the 
name  being  given  to  them  on  account  of  their  paler  colour.  The 
third,  lateral  division  Pt,  is  called  the  putamen.  The  use  of  these 
two  names  for  the  two  different  parts  of  the  one  body,  appears  to 
be  justified  by  the  different  connections  and  features  of  the  two 
parts. 

The  grey  mass  which  in  a  horizontal  section  (Fig.  115,  Nc)  is 
separated  from  the  nucleus  lenticularis  by  the  front  limb  of  the 
external  capsule,  and  which  projects  into  the  lateral  ventricle,  is 
called  the  nucleus  caudatus.  The  nucleus  caudatus  and  the  nucleus 
lenticularis  form  together  the  corpus  striatum ;  the  former,  since 
it  projects  into  the  lateral  ventricle,  being  the  part  of  the  corpus 
striatum  seen  when  the  lateral  ventricle  is  laid  open,  is  sometimes 
spoken  of  as  the  intraventricular  portion  of  the  whole  body,  while 
the  nucleus  lenticularis,  which  is  wholly  hiden  in  the  hemisphere 
and  in  no  part  projects  into  the  lateral  ventricle,  is  called  the 
extraventricular  portion. 


CHAP,  ii.] 


THE   BRAIN. 


975 


But  only  a  part,  indeed  only  a  relatively  small  part,  of  the 
nucleus  caudatus  is  disclosed  in  such  a  horizontal  section ;  to  learn 
the  somewhat  peculiar  form  and  relations  of  the  whole  nucleus  a 


FlG.    117.       DIAGRAMMATIC  OUTLINE  OF  A  TRANSVERSE  DORSOVENTRAL  SECTION  THROUGH 

THE  EIGHT  HEMISPHERE  (MAN)  AT  A  LEVEL  ANTERIOR  TO  FIG.  116.     (Natural 
size.)     (Sherrington.) 

Nc.  nucleus  caudatus ;  Gp',  Gp",  globus  pallidus,  seen  here  in  two  segments,  and 
Ft.  putamen  of  nucleus  lenticularis ;  OT.  optic  thalamus  with  ca.  anterior 
commissure  in  close  relation  to  da,  anterior  limb  of  internal  capsule. 
ce.  external  capsule,  op.  optic  tract,  cc.  corpus  callosum,  /.  fornix.  Iv.  a 
space  that  in  its  upper  part  belongs  to  the  lateral  ventricle,  in  its  lower  was 
filled  by  the  fold  of  subarachnoid  tissue  and  pia  mater  the  side  fringe  of  which, 
covered  with  epithelium,  forms  the  choroid  plexus ;  this  fold  was  detached  in 
the  making  of  the  section  and  was  removed.  In.  the  insula ;  F.  Frontal  lobe ; 
P.  Parietal  lobe ;  T.  Temporal  lobe. 

For  greater  clearness,  the  cortical  grey  matter,  which  is  shaded  in  Fig.  116,  is  in 
this  figure  left  unshaded. 


F. 


62 


976 


CORPUS   STRIATUM. 


[BOOK  in 


number  of  sections  of  a  hemisphere  taken  in  different  planes  must 
be  studied ;  and  these  will  at  the  same  time  explain  why  the  nucleus 
is  called  '  caudatus.'  These  teach  us  that  the  nucleus  has  somewhat 
the  form  of  a  comma  (Fig.  119).  The  thick  rounded  head  forms 
the  lateral  wall  of  the  front  part  of  the  lateral  ventricle ;  thence 
the  body  passes  backward  narrowing  rapidly  and  diverging  some- 
what laterally ;  in  its  course  it  arches  over  the  nucleus  lenticularis, 
curving  so  much  that  the  end  of  the  tail  sweeping  round  the  hinder 
border  of  that  body  and  changing  its  direction  runs  eventually 
ventral  to  it.  In  a  horizontal  section  taken  at  a  certain  depth 
such  as  that  represented  in  Fig.  115  only  a  portion  of  the  head  or 
body  (Nc)  in  the  front  part  of  the  figure,  and  a  transverse  section 
of  the  end  of  the  tail  (Nc)  in  the  hind  part  of  the  figure  are  seen  ; 
all  the  intervening  portion  of  the  nucleus  lies  above  the  plane  of 


FIG.  118.  DIAGRAMMATIC  OUTLINE  OF  A  TRANSVERSE  DORSOVENTRAL  SECTION  OF 
EIGHT  HEMISPHERE  (MAN)  THROUGH  THE  FRONTAL  LOBE.  (Natural  size.) 
(Sherrington.) 

Nc.  Head  of  nucleus  caudatus,  and  Nl  the  front  end  of  the  putamen  of  the  nucleus 
lenticularis  becoming  fused  with  it.  c.  c.  corpus  callosum,  cut  through  at  its 
front  bend  or  rostrum  so  that  both  dorsal  and  ventral  portions  are  shewn; 
between  these  is  seen  the  fifth  ventricle  or  cavity  in  the  septum  lucidum  SI. 
Iv.  lateral  ventricle.  Cl.  claustrum.  F.  Frontal  lobe. 

Cortical  grey  matter,  as  in  Fig.  117,  left  unshaded. 


CHAP.  IL] 


THE   BRAIN. 


977 


the  section.  In  a  transverse,  dorso- ventral,  section  taken  somewhat 
anteriorly  through  the  front  limb  of  the  capsule,  Fig.  117,  the 
head  or  body  of  the  nucleus  caudatus  (No),  which  has  not  yet 
reached  its  greatest  dimensions,  is  seen  lying  dorsal  to  the 
nucleus  lenticularis,  separated  from  it  by  the  white  mass  of  the 
front  lirnb  (cm)  of  the  capsule,  though  this  is  somewhat  broken 
up  by  strands  of  grey  matter  passing  from  one  nucleus  to  the 
other.  In  a  transverse,  dorso-ventral  section,  taken  still  more 
anteriorly,  through  the  frontal  lobe  (Fig.  118),  the  head  of  the 
nucleus  caudatus  is  seen  at  about  its  greatest  size,  and  the 
diminishing  nucleus  lenticularis  (Nl),  represented  by  the  putamen 
alone,  is  becoming  fused  with  it,  the  two  nuclei  being  separated 
by  a  small  quantity  of  white  matter  of  the  internal  capsule  and 
that  largely  broken  up  by  bridles  of  grey  matter,  giving  rise  to  a 
striated  appearance.  In  a  similar  section  still  farther  forward,  the 
nucleus  lenticularis  would  be  absent,  the  head  of  the  nucleus 
caudatus  appearing  by  itself.  Returning  to  the  hinder  part  of 
the  hemisphere,  we  find  in  a  dorso-ventral  section  taken  through 


Fro.  119.  DIAGRAMMATIC  OUTLINE  OP  A  SAGITTAL  SECTION  TAKEN  THROUGH  THE 
EIGHT  HEMISPHERE  (MAN)  SEEN  FROM  THE  MESIAL  SURFACE.  (Half  Natural 
size.)  (Sherrington.) 

The  plane  of  the  section  is  not  truly  sagittal,  but  slightly  inclined. 

Nc,  the  caudate  nucleus  exposed,  to  the  left  of  the  letters  Nc  in  nearly  its  entire 
anterior  extent,  to  right  of  the  letters  in  a  considerable  part  of  its  posterior 
extent.  It  forms  an  arch  of  grey  matter  over  the  grey  matter  of  Pt  the 
putamen  and  Gp  the  globus  pallidus  of  the  lenticular  nucleus.  Na,  the 
amygdaloid  nucleus.  Ci,  Ci,  Gi,  the  internal  capsule;  Ca,  the  anterior 
commissure;  cc,  the  hinder  limit  of  fibres  of  the  splenium  corporis  callosi. 
P.  the  parietal  lobe;  T.  the  temporal. 

62—2 


978  CORPUS   STRIATUM.  [BOOK  in. 

the  hind  limb  of  the  capsule,  Fig.  116,  that  while  the  nucleus 
lenticularis  is  here  at  its  greatest  size,  the  head  of  the  nucleus 
caudatus  (Nc),  lying  dorsal  to  the  nucleus  lenticularis  and  sepa- 
rated from  it  by  a  considerable  thickness  of  internal  capsule,  has 
much  diminished ;  the  same  section  moreover  shews,  ventral  to 
the  nucleus  lenticularis  and  clinging  to  the  descending  horn  of 
the  lateral  ventricle  (l.v.d.),  the  extreme  tip  of  tbe  tail  of  the 
nucleus  caudatus  (Nc)  soon  about  to  fuse  with  the  small  mass  of 
grey  matter  called  the  nucleus  amygdalae  (No).  A  sagittal 
{longitudinal  dorso-ventral)  section  taken  at  some  distance  from 
the  median  line  (Fig.  119),  shews  the  curved  course  of  the  larger 
portion  of  the  nucleus  caudatus,  the  extreme  head  as  well  as  the 
latter  part  of  the  tail  lying  out  of  the  plane  of  the  section ;  and  a 
similar  section  taken  nearer  the  middle  line  (Fig.  122)  shews  how 
the  nucleus  in  the  middle  portion  is  broken  up  by  bands  of 
fibres  of  the  internal  capsule  traversing  it,  and  thus  contributing 
to  the  striated  appearance ;  the  same  section  also  shews  that  the 
globus  pallidus  as  well  as  the  putamen  becomes  continuous  with 
the  nucleus  caudatus. 

Thus  when  we  speak  of  the  corpus  striatum  as  a  whole  we 
mean  a  large  mass  of  grey  matter  lying  lateral  to  the  optic  thala- 
mus,  reaching  nearly  as  far  back  as  that  body  and  stretching  much 
farther  forward,  as  far  forward  in  fact  as  does  the  lateral  ventricle; 
but  it  is  important  to  remember  that  it  is  divided  into  two 
masses  or  nuclei,  which  are  fused  together  and  that  imperfectly 
at  the  very  front  only.  These  two  nuclei  are,  the  one  the  comma 
shaped  nucleus  caudatus  the  bulb  of  which  is  placed  forward 
projecting  into  the  lateral  ventricle,  and  which  on  the  whole  is 
the  more  dorsal  portion  of  the  whole  body,  the  other  the 
irregularly  shaped  nucleus  lenticularis  the  bulk  of  which  is 
placed  farther  back  than  the  lateral  ventricle,  by  the  side  of  the 
optic  thalamus,  and  which  on  the  whole  is  the  more  ventral 
portion  of  the  whole  body.  It  is  no  less  important  to  remember 
that  the  radiating  fibres,  which  we  call  the  internal  capsule,  pass 
in  the  hinder  region  of  the  whole  body  between  the  thalamus 
and  the  nucleus  lenticularis,  forming  the  hind  limb  of  the  capsule, 
and  in  the  front  region  between  the  nucleus  caudatus  and  the 
nucleus  lenticularis  forming  the  front  limb  of  the  capsule,  the 
front  and  hind  limbs  being  bent  on  each  other  so  as  to  form  an 
angle,  the  so-called  knee. 

§  628.  The  optic  thalamus  as  a  whole  is  a  somewhat  oval 
mass  of  grey  matter,  lying  as  we  have  said  athwart  the  diverging 
crus,  in  which  it  is  partly  imbedded.  Its  curved  median  side 
covered  with  a  thin  layer  of  central  grey  matter  forms  the  lateral 
wall  of  the  third  ventricle  (Figs.  115,  116,  121),  and  in  a  longi- 
tudinal vertical  section  of  the  brain  taken  in  the  line  of  the  middle 
of  the  third  ventricle  (Fig.  120,  O.T.)  is  seen  occupying  the  space 
between  the  fornix  and  hind  end  (splenium)  of  the  corpus  callosum 


CHAP.  IL] 


979 


FIG.  120.  VIEW  OF  EIGHT  HALF  OF  BRAIN  OF  MAN,  AS  DISCLOSED  BY  A  LONGI- 
TUDINAL SECTION  IN  THE  MEDIAN  LINE  THROUGH  THE  LONGITUDINAL  FISSURE. 
(Half  natural  size.)  (Sherrington.) 

The  lull},  seen  in  longitudinal  section  at  B,  passes  into  the  pom  P,  and  into  the 
cms  cerebri,  which  last  is  cut  obliquely  across  as  it  diverges  into  the  hemisphere 
and  passes  out  of  the  section.  A  part  of  the  ventral  surface  of  the  crus  is 
shewn  in  the  shaded  part  marked  C.  R.  At  GL  the  central  canal  of  the  spinal 
cord  is  seen  opening  out  into  the  fourth  ventricle  (4th)  overhung  by  the  cere- 
bellum (bisected  in  the  middle  line),  and  passing  on  by  the  aqueduct  beneath  the 
posterior,  Q.P,  and  anterior,  Q.A,  corpora  quadrigemina  into  the  third  ventricle 
(3).  The  posterior  corpus  quadrigeminum  is  continuous  behind  with  the  valve 
of  Vieussens,  attached  to  the  superior  peduncle  of  the  cerebellum,  and  seen  in 
a  longitudinal  section  overhanging  the  front  part  of  the  fourth  ventricle.  The 
corpora  quadrigemina  appear  relatively  small  because  the  section  passes  in  the 
median  line  in  the  depression  between  the  right  and  left  bodies  of  the  two 
pairs;  and  immediately  in  front  of  them  is  the  section  of  the  mesially  placed 
pineal  gland  P,  which  overhangs  the  opening  of  the  aqueduct  into  the  third 
ventricle,  and  the  right  arm  of  which  running  in  the  lateral  wall  of  the  third 
ventricle  is  shewn  by  an  unshaded  tract. 

The  roof  of  the  third  ventricle  is  seen  to  be  furnished  by  the  arch  of  the  fornix  F,  shewn 
unshaded  in  longitudinal  section.  Posteriorly  the  body  of  the  fornix  passes 
into  the  diverging  right  posterior  pillar,  where  F  is  shaded,  and  is  lost  to  view 
under  the  overhanging  rounded  hind  end  or  splenium  Sp.  of  the  corpus  callosum. 
In  front  the  body  of  the  fornix  is  seen  passing  just  behind  the  transverse  section 
of  the  anterior  commissure  A ,  into  the  diverging  right  anterior  pillar,  f,  which 
is  lost  to  view  as  it  stretches  in  the  lateral  wall  of  the  ventricle  towards  the 
corpus  mammillare  or  albicans  M.  The  small  white  cross  immediately  behind 
/,  indicates  the  position  of  the  foramen  of  Monro.  The  bulging  median  surface 
of  the  optic  thalamus,  O.T,  is  seen  forming  the  lateral  wall  of  the  hinder  (and, 
owing  to  the  cranial  flexure,  the  more  dorsal)  part  of  the  third  ventricle,  and 
on  this  below  the  area  of  the  pineal  gland  is  seen,  unshaded,  the  section  of 
the  soft  or  middle  commissure  C.  Between  the  pineal  gland  (P)  and  the 
splenium  Sp,  is  seen  the  hind  end  orpulvinar  of  the  thalamus  projecting  into 


980  OPTIC   THALAMUS.  [BOOK  m. 

the  so-called  transverse  fissure  of  the  brain,  shewn  shaded  in  the  figure,  by 
which  the  pia  mater  passing  on  beneath  the  posterior  part  of  the  cerebrum  and 
above  the  cerebellum  gains  access  to  the  third  ventricle,  the  position  of  the 
velum  being  shewn  by  the  thin  black  line  stretching  from  the  splenium  to  the 
fornix.  The  front  (and  more  ventral)  part  of  the  third  ventricle  is  seen  to  end 
in  the  infundibulum  attached  to  which  is  the  pituitary  body  Ht  seen  in  section 
at  L.  In  front  of  the  infundibulum  is  seen  the  optic  nerve  cut  across  at  the 
optic  decussation  OP,  stretching  from  which  to  the  anterior  commissure  is  the 
lamina  terminalis. 

Stretching  between  the  corpus  callosum  c.c.  (seen  in  longitudinal  section  with  a 
striated  appearance  and  ending  in  front  at  the  rostrum  R.  and  behind  at  the 
splenium  Sp.)  dorsally  and  the  fornix  ventrally  is  seen  (unshaded)  the  septum 
lucidum  S.L,  but  the  greater  part  of  this  has  been  cut  away  in  order  to  disclose 
the  right  lateral  ventricle  in  the  wall  of  which  is  seen  the  bulging  nucleus 
caudatus  N.C. 

Above  the  corpus  callosum  is  seen  the  mesial  surface  of  the  right  hemisphere 
forming  the  right  lateral  wall  of  the  longitudinal  fissure.  On  this  mesial 
surface  appears  immediately  above  the  corpus  callosum  the  arched  gyrus 
fornicatus  G.F,  defined  above  by  the  callo so -marginal  fissure  /.cm.  The  whole 
of  the  surface  seen  in  the  frontal  region  in  front  of  the  calloso-marginal  fissure, 
though  divided  by  fissures,  is  called  the  marginal  convolution.  In  the  middle 
parietal  region  a  block  of  the  cerebral  substance  has  been  removed  in  order  to 
shew  the  position  of  the  central  fissure  or  fissure  of  Rolando,  f.c.,  and 
immediately  below  this  is  seen  a  part  of  PA.C  the  paracentral  lobule.  In 
the  occipital  region  PR.C.  is  the  precuneus  or  quadrate  lobule,  and  C,  the 
cuneus,  while  at  G.L  is  seen  a  part  of  the  lingual  lobule.  T.i  is  a  part  of  the 
inferior  temporo-occipital  convolution,  the  greater  part  of  which  is  hidden  to 
view  by  the  pons  and  crus. 

above,  and  the  diverging  crus  below.  Its  more  or  less  straight 
lateral  border  abuts  on  the  internal  capsule  (Figs.  115,  116,  121). 
Its  dorsal  surface,  as  we  have  already  seen,  also  forms  part  of  the 
wall  of  the  third  ventricle  and  is  free ;  but  there  lies  close  above 
it  the  prolongation  of  the  pia  mater,  forming  the  velum  inter- 
positum  with  its  choroid  plexus  (§  602),  which  creeps  in  over  it 
beneath  the  projecting  hind  end  of  the  corpus  callosum  and 
the  fornix  (Fig.  120).  Its  ventral  surface  is  fused  with  the  crus; 
indeed  the  tegmental  or  dorsal  portion  of  the  crus  may  be  said 
to  end  in  it  and  in  certain  structures  lying  ventral  to  the 
thalamus,  in  what  is  called  the  "subthalamic  region"  (Fig.  116), 
while  the  fibres  of  the  pes  pass  first  ventral  and  then  lateral  to 
it  to  form  the  internal  capsule. 

The  grey  matter  of  the  whole  body  is  more  or  less  distinctly 
divided  by  sheets  of  white  matter,  as  seen  both  in  horizontal  and 
in  vertical  sections  (Figs.  115,  116,  121),  into  three  parts  which 
have  received  the  name  of  nuclei,  namely  the  median  or  inner 
nucleus  (Fig.  116,  in)  which  with  the  thin  layer  of  central  grey 
matter  forms  the  side  wall  of  the  third  ventricle,  the  larger  lateral 
nucleus  (In)  which  abuts  upon  the  internal  capsule,  and  the  small 
anterior  nucleus  (an)  which  lies  on  the  dorsal  surface  of  the  front 
part  of  the  body  and  which  thus  at  its  front  end  appears  to  project 
into  the  lateral  ventricle. 

These  three  nuclei  form  however  not  the  whole  of  the  optic 
thalamus,  but  only  the  larger  front  portion ;  behind  them  lies  the 


CHAP,  ii.]  THE   BRAIN.  981 

important  portion  called  the  pulvinar  y  into  which  the  hind  part  of 
the  median  nucleus  merges ;  this  is  partly  imbedded  in  the  crus 
ventrally  and  in  the  hemisphere  laterally,  and  is  partly  free, 
coming  to  the  surface  beneath  the  hind  end  of  the  corpus  callosum. 
In  a  median  longitudinal  section  of  the  brain  (Fig.  120),  it  is  the 
pulvinar  which  forms  the  cushion-like  (hence  the  name)  end  of  the 
thalamus  beneath  the  overhanging  splenium  of  the  corpus  cal- 
losum, by  the  side  of  the  pineal  gland ;  and  in  the  horizontal  view 
(Fig.  115,  Pvr),  in  which  the  hemispheres  are  supposed  to  have 
been  removed,  the  same  pulvinar  is  seen  projecting  over  the  crus 
by  the  side  of  the  anterior  corpus  quadrigeminum.  The  buried 
portion  of  the  pulvinar  is  exposed  in  a  transverse  section  taken 
through  the  anterior  corpus  quadrigeminum,  Fig.  114;  the  extreme 
end  of  this  part  of  the  pulvinar  (Pvr)  is  here  seen  lying  dorsal  and 
lateral  to  the  pes  of  the  crus,  immediately  above  two  masses  of 
grey  matter,  the  corpora  geniculata  (Ggl.  Ggm.),  of  which  we  shall 
speak  later  on.  One  of  these,  the  lateral  corpus  geniculatum 
(C.g.l.),  is  especially  connected  with  ;bhe  optic  tract  (op),  and,  as 
we  shall  see  hereafter,  the  pulvinar  itself  is  also  connected  with 
the  optic  tract  and  is  an  important  part  of  the  central  apparatus 
of  vision. 

§  629.  The  substantia  nigra,  the  red  nucleus  and  other  grey 
matter  of  the  tegmentum.  Nerve-cells  and  groups  of  nerve-cells, 
or  areas  of  grey  matter,  too  small  to  deserve  special  names, 
are  scattered  throughout  the  tegmentum  along  its  course.  But, 
besides  these  and  the  nuclei  of  the  third  and  fourth  cranial  nerves, 
of  which  we  have  already  spoken,  certain  larger  collections  of  grey 
matter  deserve  attention.  A  conspicuous  mass  of  grey  matter, 
circular  in  transverse  section,  placed  in  the  midst  of  the  tegmentum 
on  each  side  but  somewhat  near  the  middle  line,  and  stretching 
from  the  hinder  margin  of  the  third  ventricle  beneath  the  anterior 
corpus  quadrigeminum  (Figs.  114,  115),  is,  from  the  red  tint  it 
possesses,  called  the  red  nucleus,  nucleus  or  locus  ruber.  It  is 
traversed  by  fibres  of  the  third  nerve  as  these  make  their  way 
ventrally  from  the  nucleus  to  the  surface. 

We  must  consider  also  as  belonging  to  the  tegmentum  a 
large  area  of  grey  matter,  somewhat  lens-shaped  in  section  (Fig. 
114,  Sn)  which  lies  between  the  pes  and  tegmentum,  sharply 
marking  off  the  one  from  the  other.  From  its  dark  appearance 
due  to  the  abundance  of  black  pigment  it  is  called  the  substantia 
nigra  or  locus  niger.  It  acquires  its  largest  dimensions  at  about 
the  middle  of  the  length  of  the  crus,  coming  to  an  end  in  front 
(Fig.  116,  Sri)  and  fading  away  behind  (Fig.  113)  as  the  crus 
passes  beneath  the  posterior  corpora  quadrigemina.  These  two, 
the  red  nucleus  and  the  substantia  nigra,  are  perhaps  the  most 
important  collections  of  grey  matter  in  the  tegmentum,  but  we 
may  add  that  at  the  front  of  the  crus  as  the  substantia  nigra 
comes  to  an  end  there  is  seen  in  a  somewhat  similar  position 


982  INTERMEDIATE   GREY   MATTER.  [BOOK  in. 

ventral  to  the  hind  part  of  the  optic  thalamus  a  collection  of  grey 
matter  called  the  corpus  subthalamicum  (Fig.  116,  C.sb). 

At  the  hinder  part  of  the  cms,  as  it  is  about  to  plunge  into 
the  pons,  while  the  pes,  now  decreasing  relatively  in  size,  still  con- 
tinues to  be  ordinary  white  matter  composed  of  longitudinal 
bundles  of  medullated  fibres,  the  tegmentum  takes  on  more  and 
more  the  structure  which  in  speaking  of  the  bulb  we  called 
reticular  formation,  and  which,  as  we  saw,  deserves  to  be  con- 
sidered as  a  kind  of  grey  matter. 

The  grey  matter  of  the  pons.  When  the  conjoined  crura  as  we 
trace  them  backward  plunge  beneath  the  pons  the  longitudinal 
fibres  of  the  pes  of  each  crus  are  as  we  have  said  soon  split  up 
into  bundles  scattered  among  the  transverse  fibres  belonging  to 
the  pons  itself.  Dorsal  to  this  system  of  transverse  and  longi- 
tudinal fibres  forming  the  pons  proper,  between  it  on  the  ventral 
surface  and  the  central  grey  matter  with  the  posterior  corpora 
quadrigemina  on  the  dorsal  surface,  is  a  region  which  may  be 
called  tegmental  since  it  is  a  continuation  of  the  tegmentum  of 
the  crus.  In  the  front  part  of  the  pons  (Fig.  113),  where  the 
posterior  corpora  quadrigemina  still  form  the  dorsal  roof  of  the 
section,  this  tegmental  area,  which  is  much  broken  up  by  certain 
strands  of  longitudinal  fibres  of  which  we  shall  speak  later  on, 
contains  scattered  nerve  cells,  and  is  largely  composed  of  reticular 
formation.  In  this  is  placed  on  each  side  a  group  of  nerve  cells, 
the  locus  caeruleus  (Fig.  113,  I.e.),  to  which  we  have  already 
referred  (§621)  as  probably  serving  in  part  as  the  origin  of  the 
descending  root  of  the  fifth  nerve  (V.  d.),  just  ventral  to  which 
it  lies.  This  acquires  larger  dimensions  farther  back,  in  the 
front  part  of  the  fourth  ventricle  (Fig.  115,  I.e.)  between  the  levels 
represented  in  Figs.  112  and  113,  and  is  a  collection  of  large 
spindle-shaped  nerve  cells;  it  has  a  bluish  tint  when  its  black 
pigment  is  seen  shining  through  the  surrounding  more  or  less 
transparent  material,  hence  the  name. 

In  the  hinder  parts  of  the  pons  (Figs.  Ill,  112)  where  the 
cerebellum  is  seen  overhanging  the  open  fourth  ventricle,  the 
reticular  formation  of  the  tegmental  area  is  still  more  conspicuous. 
The  only  special  collection  of  grey  matter  in  this  region  to  which 
we  need  call  attention  is  one  which,  consisting  like  the  olivary 
body  of  the  bulb  (or  inferior  olive)  of  a  wall  of  grey  matter 
surrounding  and  surrounded  by  white  matter,  is  called  the  upper 
olive  (Figs.  Ill,  112,  s.o.). 

The  ventral  part  of  the  pons,  or  the  pons  proper,  unlike  the  pes 
of  the  crus,  contains  mixed  with  the  fibres  a  very  considerable 
quantity  of  grey  matter.  This  is  fairly  abundant  in  the  front  part 
of  the  pons  (Fig.  113)  below  the  corpora  quadrigemina  but  in- 
creases even  more  behind  this  (Figs.  Ill,  112).  Hence  though  the 
pons  proper  is  largely  built  up  of  transverse  and  longitudinal 
fibres,  and  though  it  contains  no  compact  aggregations  of  grey 


CHAP.  IL]  THE   BRAIN.  98S 

matter  receiving  special  names,  it  does  contain  scattered  through- 
out it  a  very  large  quantity  of  grey  matter,  far  more  indeed  than 
is  present  in  the  tegmental  portion ;  the  grey  matter  of  the  pons, 
that  is  of  the  pons  proper,  must  be  regarded  as  forming  a  very 
important  part  of  the  grey  matter  of  the  crural  system,  and  of 
no  little  physiological  significance. 

Behind  the  pons  the  crural  system  is  continued  into  the  bulb, 
with  whose  structure  we  have  already  dealt. 

4.     Other  Collections  of  Grey  Matter. 

§  630.  Of  these,  three  deserve  chief  attention,  and  may  be- 
classed  together,  though  they  differ  in  nature. 

The  grey  matter  of  the  corpora  quadrigemina.  On  each  side  of 
and  somewhat  dorsal  to  the  central  grey  matter  of  the  aqueduct 
which,  as  we  have  seen,  is  well  developed  especially  on  the  ventral 
side,  collections  of  grey  matter  form  the  chief  part  of  the  corpora 
quadrigemina,  both  anterior  and  posterior. 

The  grey  matter  of  the  anterior  corpora  quadrigemina  (Fig. 
114,  A.  Q.  n)  is  more  distinctly  marked  off  from,  and  separated  by 
a  wider  tract  of  white  matter  from  the  central  grey  matter  of  the 
aqueduct  than  is  that  of  the  posterior  corpora  quadrigemina  (Fig, 

113,  nPQ)\  it  is  moreover  of  a  different  nature.     Indeed  the  two 
pairs   of  bodies   have   quite   different  relations,  are  of  different 
nature,  and  perform  different  functions. 

Corpora  geniculata.  The  two  optic  nerves,  as  we  shall  see  in 
detail  later  on,  give  rise,  through  the  optic  decussation,  to  the  two 
optic  tracts.  Each  optic  tract  (Figs.  108,  114,  Op),  winds  round 
the  crus  cerebri  on  its  ventral  surface  to  reach  the  substance  of 
the  hemisphere  in  the  region  below  the  optic  thalamus,  and  as  it 
does  so  is  described  as  dividing  into  a  lateral  and  median  portion. 
The  lateral  portion  just  as  it  sweeps  round  the  far  edge,  that  is 
the  outer  or  lateral  edge,  of  the  crus  bears  a  rounded  swelling 
(Figs.  108  B  and  C,  C.gl.)  the  lateral  or  outer  corpus  genicu- 
latum,  the  interior  of  which  consists  largely  of  grey  matter  (Fig. 

114,  Cgl).      The   median    portion    similarly   bears    another    like 
swelling  occupying  a  more  median  position,  the  median  or  inner 
corpus   geniculatum   (Fig.  108  A  and  B,  Cgm),   the   interior   of 
which  (Fig.  114,  Cgm)  also  consists  of  grey  matter.     It  is  to  be 
regretted  that  these  two  bodies  should  bear  the  same  name,  for 
they  are  different  in  their  origin,  in  their  connections,  and  in  their 
functions.     The  lateral  body  is  said  to  be  derived  from  the  fore- 
brain,  that  is  from  the  vesicle  of  the  third  ventricle,  has  definite 
connections  with  the  retinal  optic  fibres,  and  is  distinctly  con- 
cerned in  vision ;  the  median  body  is  derived  from  the  midbrain, 
is  not  definitely  connected  with  the  retinal  fibres,  and  appears  to 
be  in  no  way  concerned  in  vision.     We  shall  however  return  later 
on  to  the  connections  and  probable  functions  of  these  bodies. 


984  THE   FIBRES   OF   THE   BRAIN.  [BOOK  HI. 

Corpus  dentatum  of  the  cerebellum.  In  the  midst  of  the  mass 
of  white  matter  which  is  formed  in  the  interior  of  the  cerebellum  by 
the  confluence  of  the  three  peduncles,  is  found  (Fig.  Ill,  CD)  an 
area  of  grey  matter  arranged,  like  the  olivary  body  of  the  bulb,  as 
a  sharply  folded  or  plaited  band  in  the  shape  of  a  flask  or  bowl. 
As  in  the  similar  olivary  body  the  grey  wall  of  the  flask  is 
covered  up  by  and  its  interior  filled  up  with  white  matter; 
the  mouth  of  the  flask  is,  on  each  side,  directed  towards  the 
median  line;  the  fibres  pass  chiefly  to  the  superior  peduncle. 

There  are  also  other  collections  of  grey  matter  in  the  central 
white  matter  of  the  cerebellum,  one  of  which,  called  the  "  nucleus 
of  the  roof,"  is  connected  with  the  two  inferior  peduncles. 


The  Arrangement  of  the  Fibres  of  the  Brain. 

§  631.  The  systems,  tracts  and  bundles  of  fibres  in  which  the 
white  matter  of  the  brain  is  arranged,  may  be  distinguished  from 
each  other,  partly  through  mere  mechanical  separation  by  means 
-of  the  scalpel,  partly  by  being  traced  out  with  the  help  of  the 
microscope,  but,  as  in  the  spinal  cord,  much  more  fully  and 
completely  by  differences  of  development,  and  by  the  method  of 
degeneration. 

We  have  seen  that  a  marked  feature  of  the  brain  is  presented 
by  the  two  crura  cerebri  which,  running  forward  from  the  hind 
parts  of  the  brain,  spread  out  into  each  cerebral  hemisphere. 
We  have  also  seen  that  the  crus  in  the  wide  sense  of  the  word 
consists  of  two  parts,  a  dorsal  part,  the  tegmentum,  and  a  ventral 
part,  the  pes  or  crusta,  and  that  these  two  parts  differ  very  strik- 
ingly from  each  other  in  structure  and  in  relations.  The  pes 
consists  exclusively  of  bundles  of  longitudinal  fibres,  and  we  may 
trace  these  from  the  cerebral  hemispheres  into  the  pons  and  some 
of  them  beyond  the  pons  into  the  bulb  and  spinal  cord.  The  teg- 
mentum is  more  complex  in  structure ;  it  consists  of  grey  matter, 
and  of  fibres  and  bundles  of  fibres  having  various  relations,  both 
with  the  collections  of  grey  matter  lying  within  itself  and  with 
surrounding  structures.  It  too  has  connections  with  the  parts 
lying  in  front  of  it,  and  with  the  parts  lying  behind  it ;  we  may 
trace  it  too  backwards  through  the  pons  into  the  bulb  and  forwards 
to  the  optic  thalamus.  If  we  allow  ourselves  to  conceive  of  the 
optic  thalamus  as  constituting  the  front  ending  of  the  tegmentum, 
we  may  arrange  a  large  part  of  the  brain  into  two  main  regions, 
into  a  tegmental  region  stretching  from  the  optic  thalamus  through 
the  dorsal  portion  of  the  pons  to  the  dorsal  portion  of  the  bulb, 
and  into  a  region,  which  we  may  call  the  pedal  region,  stretching 
from  the  internal  capsule  through  the  ventral  portion  of  the  pons 
to  the  ventral  portion  of  the  bulb. 


CHAP.  IL] 


THE   BRAIN. 


985 


FIG.  121.     OUTLINE  OF  HORIZONTAL  SECTION  OF  BRAIN,  TO  SHEW  THE  INTERNAL 
CAPSULE.     (Natural  size.) 

The  section  is  taken  at  a  level  more  ventral  than  shewn  in  Fig.  115.  The  grey 
matter  of  the  cortex  and  claustrum  is  left  unshaded,  but  that  of  the  corpus 
striatum  and  optic  thalamus  is  shaded. 

OT.  optic  thalamus,  shewing  the  median,  lateral,  and  anterior  nuclei.  NL.  nucleus 
lenticularis,  shewing  the  putamen  large,  and  the  inner  division  of  the  globus 
pallidus  very  small.  NC.  nucleus  caudatus,  the  large  head  in  front  of,  and 
the  diminishing  tail  behind,  the  thalamus. 

G.  the  knee  of  the  internal  capsule.  From  '  Eye'  to  'Dig,'  marks  the  position  of 
the  pyramidal  tract  as  a  whole,  and  the  several  letters  indicate  broadly  the 
relative  positions  of  the  several  constituents  of  the  tract,  named  according  to 
the  movements  with  which  they  are  concerned;  thus  Eye  movements  of  the 
eyes ;  Hd.  of  the  head ;  Tg.  of  the  tongue ;  mth.  of  the  mouth ;  shl.  of  the 
shoulder;  elb.  of  the  elbow;  Dig.  of  the  hand;  Abdr-ef  the  abdomen;  Hip.  of 
the  hip ;  Kn.  of  the  knee ;  Dig.  of  the  foot. 

S.  the  temporo-occipital  tract,  oc.  fibres  to  the  occipital  lobe.  Op.  optic  radiation. 
At  this  level  the  fibres  of  the  frontal  tract,  in  the  fore  limb  of  the  capsule  in 


986 


THE   FIBRES   OF   THE   BRAIN. 


[BOOK  in. 


front  of  the  pyramidal  tract,  run  almost  horizontally,  parallel  with  the  plane 
of  the  section.     Of.  Fig.  122,  Fron. 

cc.  the  rostrum  of  the  corpus  callosum,  Spl.  the  splenium  of  the  same,  both  cut 
across  horizontally.  The  thick  dark  line  indicates  the  boundary  of  the 
cavities  of  the  anterior  and  descending  horns  of  the  lateral  ventricle  and  of 
the  third  ventricle,  the  two  ventricles  being  laid  open  into  one  by  the  removal 
of  the  velum  and  choroid  plexus  &c.  The  oval  outline  in  the  fore  part  of  this 
cavity  indicates  the  fornix. 

Lateral  to  the  nucleus  lenticularis  is  seen  in  outline  the  claustrum,  the  cortex  of 
the  island  of  Beil  and  the  operculum  or  convolution  overlapping  the  island 
of  Eeil. 

P  is  inserted  to  shew  which  is  the  hind  part  of  the  section. 

The  fibres  of  the  brain  as  a  whole  may  be  broadly  classified 
into  longitudinal  tracts  connecting  parts  of  the  brain  with  suc- 
ceeding parts  and  into  transverse  or  commissural  tracts  between 
one  lateral  half  and  the  other,  and  into  tracts  connected  with  the 
several  cranial  nerves.  Taking  the  longitudinal  fibres  first  we 
may  in  accordance  with  the  division  just  explained  into  a  pedal 


dors.lum 


FIG.  122. 


OUTLINE  OF  A  SAGITTAL  SECTION  THROUGH  THE  HEMISPHERE. 
(Sherrington.) 


Man. 


The  section  is  taken  not  far  to  the  right  of  the  median  plane  and  is  one  half  linear 
of  natural  size.  The  grey  matter  of  the  corpus  striatum  an£  thalamus  is 
shaded. 

Nc,  Nc,  the  caudate  nucleus ;  Pt,  the  putamen  and  Gp,  the  globus  pallidus  of  the 
lenticular  nucleus;  OT,  the  optic  thalamus;  CI,  the  internal  capsule  with 
a  streaked  appearance  revealing  approximately  the  direction  taken  by  fibre  - 
bundles  passing  into  it  from  the  portion  of  corona  radiata  over  it.  In  these 
sets  of  bundles  may  be  broadly  distinguished  a  frontal  system,  /Von,  a  pyrami- 
dal system,  PY (sub-divisible  into  cranial  (craw.),  brachial  (brack.),  dorso-lumbar 
(dors,  lum.),  and  lumbo-sacral  (lum.  sac.),  parts)  and  a  temporo-occipital  system. 
sens.;  the  situation  of  the  genu  of  the  internal  capsule  is  indicated  by  g.  CR, 
the  crus  cerebri ;  Oc,  the  so-called  optic  radiations  passing  into  the  occipital  lobe ; 
cc,  the  splenial  end  of  the  corpus  callosum;  v,  v,  v,  the  lateral  ventricle  cut 
across  in  three  different  places;  F,  the  fornix  in  cross-section;  Op,  the  optic 
tract  in  cross-section.  Part  of  the  cerebellum  is  seen  in  outline  to  the  right. 


CHAP.  IL]  THE   BRAIN.  987 

and  a  tegmental  region,  consider  these  as  forming  on  the  one 
hand  a  pedal,  and  on  the  other  hand  a  tegmental  system. 

Both  systems  begin  as  we  shall  see  in  the  cortex  of  the 
cerebral  hemispheres.  We  shall  have  to  deal  with  the  topography 
of  the  cortex  later  on>  but  may  here  say  that  the  first  broad 
division  of  the  whole  surface  of  a  hemisphere  is  into  four  main 
regions:  frontal,  parietal,  occipital  and  temporal  (Figs.  116,  117, 
121). 

Longitudinal  fibres  of  the  Pedal  System. 

§  632.  The  pyramidal  tract.  We  have  already  (§575)  said 
that  the  pyramidal  tract  of  the  spinal  cord  may  be  traced  to  a 
particular  region  of  the  cerebral  cortex.  We  shall  study  the 
details  of  this  region,  which  is  often  spoken  of  as  the  "  motor  area" 
later  on,  but  may  here  say  that  broadly  speaking  it  is  parietal  in 
position  and  corresponds  to  the  parts  of  the  cortex  gathered  round 
the  fissure  of  Rolando.  Fibres  passing  from  the  grey  matter  of 
the  cortex  of  this  region  to  the  white  matter  below,  and  so  con- 
tributing their  share  to  the  central  white  matter  of  the  hemisphere, 
converge  (Figs.  122,  123)  to  form  part  of  the  internal  capsule, 
namely  that  part  which  in  a  horizontal  section  (Fig.  121,  Eye  to 
Dig}  occupies  the  knee  and  stretches  for  more  than  half,  or  nearly 
two-thirds,  along  the  hind  limb  of  the  capsule,  between  the  optic 
thalamus  on  the  inside  and  the  nucleus  lenticularis  on  the  outside. 
From  the  knee  and  hind  limb  of  the  capsule  they  pass  by  the 
side  of  and  ventral  to  the  optic  thalamus  (Figs.  116,  123),  and 
so  contribute  to  form  the  beginning  of  the  crus  cerebri.  In  thus 
converging  to  take  up  their  position  in  the  capsule  and  in  their 
further  passage  to  the  crus  the  fibres  follow  a  course  of  somewhat 
complicated  curvature.  As  we  trace  the  capsule  from  more  dorsal 
to  more  ventral  levels,  we  find  it  continually  changing  in  form ; 
the  exact  shape  of  the  capsule  shewn  in  Fig.  121  only  holds  good 
for  the  level  at  which  the  section  was  taken ;  it  differs  somewhat 
from  that  shewn  in  Fig.  115  taken  at  a  slightly  different  level,  and 
sections  still  more  dorsal  or  still  more  ventral  would  present  still 
greater  differences.  When  we  examine  a  series  of  horizontal  sec- 
tions, taken  in  succession  from  the  dorsal  to  the  ventral  regions, 
we  find  that  the  knee  shifts  its  position  and  changes  in  the  width 
of  its  angle,  that  the  two  limbs  vary  in  direction  in  size  and  in 
shape,  and  that  at  last  the  bent  flattened  capsule  passes  into  the 
more  or  less  rounded  crus  by  the  rapid  disappearance  of  the 
fore  limb,  and  the  consequent  extinction  of  the  angle ;  so  that  in 
one  sense  it  is  the  hind  limb  which  becomes  the  crus,  and  the 
fibres  of  the  fore  limb  may  be  said  to  pass  into  the  crus  through 
the  ventral  portion  of  the  hind  limb.  Hence  it  is  obvious  that 
the  fibres  of  the  pyramidal  tract,  like  the  other  fibres  of  the 


988 


FIBRES   OF   THE   PEDAL   SYSTEM.         [BOOK  in. 


CO.'' 


IvA 


FIG.  123. 


OUTLINE  OF  A  TRANSVERSE  DORSO-VENTRAL  SECTION  OF  THE  RIGHT 
HALF  OF  THE  BRAIN.     (Natural  size.)     (Sherrington.) 


The  section  which  is  taken  at  the  level  of  the  knee  of  the  capsule  and  is  therefore 
intermediate  between  those  shewn  in  Figs.  116 — 117  is  introduced  to  illustrate 
the  course  of  the  constituents  of  the  pyramidal  tract. 

O.T.  optic  thalamus ;  N.e.  nucleus  caudatus,  the  head  only  appears  in  this  section. 
Pt.  putamen,  Gp",  Gp'  the  two  parts  of  the  globus  pallidus  of  the  nucleus 
lenticularis ;  C.  the  claustrum;  C.E.  the  external  capsule;  In.  the  island  of 
Reil.  c.a.,  the  anterior  commissure  shaded  to  render  it  distinct  and  the  fibres 
from  the  temporo-sphenoidal  lobe  which  pass  into  it  being  indicated  by  broken 
lines.  Op.  the  optic  tract ;  Ivd.  the  end  of  the  descending  horn  of  the  lateral 
ventricle ;  F.  the  fornix ;  F'.  the  end  of  the  anterior  pillar  of  the  fornix  in  the 
base  of  the  thalamus;  c.c.  corpus  callosum  ;  OP.  anterior  part  of  the  occipital 
lobe. 

f.c.  is  the  central  fissure  or  fissure  of  Kolando.  The  course  of  the  fibres  of  the 
pyramidal  tract  connected  respectively  with  the  trunk,  leg  and  arm,  and  hence 
with  spinal  nerves,  and  of  those  connected  with  the  face  and  hence  with 
cranial  nerves,  is  shewn  by  broken  lines.  These  are  all  seen  converging  into 
the  internal  capsule  C.I.  This  figure  should  in  respect  to  the  course  of  these 
fibres  be  compared  with  the  horizontal  section  shewn  in  Fig.  121,  and  the 
sagittal  figure  shewn  in  Fig.  122. 


CHAP,  ii.]  THE   BRAIN.  989 

S.  indicates  the  course  of  the  most  anterior  and  dorsal  part  of  the  temporo- 
occipital  tract. 

The  fine  dotted  lines  converging  to  the  corpus  callosum  c.c.  indicate  the  course  of 
the  callosal  fibres. 

capsule,  are  continually  changing  their  direction  as  they  pass 
through  the  capsule.  Moreover  while  the  fibres  from  different 
parts  of  the  'motor  area'  assume  definite  positions  in  relation  to 
each  other  as  they  pass  into  the  capsule,  their  relative  positions 
are  not  constant,  but  vary  somewhat.  To  this  point  however  we 
shall  return  when  we  come  to  speak  of  the  function  of  this  tract. 

In  the  crus  these  fibres  run  exclusively  in  the  pes  and  form  a 
compact  strand  (Fig.  114,  Py}  occupying  the  central  and  larger 
portion  of  the  pes  between  a  small  median  portion  on  the  inside 
and  a  lateral  portion  on  the  outside.  Maintaining  this  position 
along  the  crus  they  enter  the  pons,  but  here  the  previously  com- 
pact strand  is  split  up,  by  the  interlacing  transverse  fibres  of  the 
pons,  into  a  number  of  scattered  bundles,  which  however  as  a 
whole  still  keep  their  central  position.  They  form  the  greater 
part  of  but  not  all  the  bundles  seen  cut  transversely  in  transverse 
sections  of  the  pons  (Figs.  112,  113).  Farther  backwards  they 
become  the  pyramid  of  the  bulb,  and  so  give  rise  in  the  spinal  cord 
to  the  direct  and  crossed  pyramidal  tracts.  These  fibres  from  the 
motor  area  of  the  cortex  of  the  cerebrum  are  thus  the  source  of  the 
pyramidal  tracts  of  the  spinal  cord,  and  hence  the  whole  strand  of 
fibres  from  the  cortex  downwards  has  been  called  the  pyramidal 
tract.  We  have  said  (§  575)  that  we  have  reasons  for  thinking 
that  the  pyramidal  tract  in  the  spinal  cord  makes  connections 
through  the  grey  matter  of  the  anterior  horn  with  the  anterior  roots 
of  all  the  spinal  nerves  in  succession ;  and  similarly  we  have  reason 
to  think  that  along  its  course  in  the  crus,  in  the  pons,  and  in  the 
bulb,  before  it  reaches  the  cord,  the  tract  also  makes  connections 
with  the  nuclei  of  those  cranial  nerves  which  are  motor  in  function. 
During  the  passage  of  the  tract  through  the  internal  capsule  the 
fibres  destined  for  cranial  nuclei  occupy  the  knee,  while  those 
belonging  to  the  spinal  cord  run  in  the  hind  limb.  Some  authors 
limit  the  term  pyramidal  tract  to  the  spinal  moiety,  since  this 
alone  forms  the  pyramid  ;  but  this  is  undesirable. 

This  tract  is  well  marked  out  by  the  degeneration  method, 
and  the  degeneration  in  it  is  a  descending  one,  the  trophic  centres 
of  the  fibres  being  cells  in  the  grey  matter  of  the  cortex.  Removal 
of  or  injury  to  the  cortex  of  the  whole  motor  area  gives  rise  to  a 
degeneration  along  the  whole  tract,  and  removal  of  or  injury  to 
part  of  the  area  gives  rise  to  degeneration  of  some  of  the  strands. 
The  tract  is  also  well  marked  out  by  the  embryological  method ; 
the  fibres  belonging  to  it  acquire  their  medulla  at  times  different 
from  those  of  other  fibres. 

'Anterior  or  frontal  cortical.  Fibres  from  the  grey  matter 
of  the  cortex  in  front  of  the  motor  area  also  pass  to  the  internal 


990  FIBRES    OF   THE   PEDAL   SYSTEM.         [BOOK  in. 

capsule,  but  occupy  the  fore  limb  (Fig.  122,  fron).  Thence  they 
pass  to  the  crus,  of  which  they  form  the  small  inner,  median 
portion  of  the  pes  (Fig.  114,  Fr.),  and  from  the  crus  pass  into  the 
pons ;  in  transverse  sections  of  the  pons  they  are  seen  as  scattered 
bundles  (Fig.  113,  F.C.)  to  the  median  side  of  the  pyramidal  fibres. 
But  here  they  seem  to  end;  the  degeneration  of  the  tract  is  a 
descending  one,  and  ceases  here.  Most  probably  the  fibres  end  in 
the  nerve  cells  of  the  grey  matter,  which  as  we  have  seen  is 
abundant  in  the  pons.  It  is  also  probable  that  through  these 
nerve  cells  the  fibres  of  this  tract  are  connected  with  transverse 
fibres  passing  along  the  middle  cerebellar  peduncle  into  the  cere- 
bellum of  the  opposite  side ;  but  this  has  not  been  definitely 
proved. 

Posterior  or  temporo- occipital  cortical.  Fibres  from. the  grey 
matter  of  parts  of  the  cortex  behind  the  motor  area  also  converge 
to  the  internal  capsule,  forming  the  hinder  end  of  the  hind  limb 
behind  the  pyramidal  tract  (Fig.  121,  S).  These  fibres  also  con- 
tribute to  form  the  crus  cerebri,  passing  into  the  pes,  of  which 
they  occupy  the  outer  lateral  portion  (Fig.  114,  Pr.O.).  From  the 
crus  they  pass  into  the  pons,  where,  like  the  fibres  of  the  pre- 
ceding tract,  they  appear  to  end,  and  probably  in  a  like  manner. 
This  fact  has  been  described  as  one  of  ascending  degeneration, 
but  in  all  probability  like  the  preceding  is  one  of  descending 
degeneration. 

The  above  three  tracts  of  fibres  may  therefore  all  be  regarded 
as  starting  from  or  having  their  trophic  centres  in  the  cortical 
grey  matter  of  the  hemispheres,  as  all  helping  to  form,  first  the 
internal  capsule  and  then  the  pes  of  the  crus  cerebri.  But  while 
•the  pyramidal  tract  passes,  in  part,  to  the  spinal  cord,  the  other 
two  cease  at  the  pons,  and  probably  through  the  grey  matter  of 
the  pons  make  connections  with  the  cerebellum.  Further  while 
the  pyramidal  tract  coming  from  the  middle  region  of  the  cortex 
occupies  a  middle  position  in  the  capsule  and  a  middle  position7 
in  the  crus,  the  system  from  the  front  part  of  the  cortex  occupied 
•a  front  position  in  the  capsule  and  an  inner  or  median  position 
in  the  crus,  and  the  system  from  the  hind  part  of  .the  cortex  a 
hind  position  in  the  capsule  and  an  outer  or  lateral  position  in 
the  crus.  As  the  three  systems  pass  from  the  cortex  through  the 
capsule  to  form  the  pes  of  the  crus,  their  positions  in  relation  to 
each  other  are  shifted  from  one  plane  into  another.  As  the  fibres 
spread  out  from  the  pes  through  the  capsule  to  all  parts  of  the 
cortex,  or,  put  in  another  way,  as  they  converge  from  the  cortex 
through  the  capsule  to  the  pes,  they  form  a  fan,  the  corona  radiata, 
which  is  not  only  curved,  but  the  constituent  parts  of  which  cross 
each  other. 

Besides  these  three  systems  all  passing  from  various  regions  of 
the  cortex  to  the  crus,  there  is  yet  a  fourth  strand  contributed  to 
the  pes  by  the  cerebral  hemisphere  though  not  starting  in  the 


CHAP,  ii.]  THE   BRAIN.  991 

cortex.  From  the  nucleus  caudatus  fibres  pass  down  to  the  crus, 
and  take  up  a  position  in  the  pes  dorsal  to  the  tracts  just  men- 
tioned, occupying  a  lens-shaped  area  immediately  ventral  to  the 
substantia  nigra,  and  probably  passing  into  the  substantia  nigra 
itself.  These  cannot  be  traced  farther  down  than  the  pons,  where 
they  appear  to  end,  though  possibly  some  terminate  higher  up 
in  the  substantia  nigra.  This  tract  has  a  descending  degene- 
ration, and  may  be  regarded  as  a  tract  analogous  to  the  front  and 
hind  cortical  tracts,  though  it  begins  not  in  the  cortex  but  in 
the  nucleus  caudatus  ;  it  is  not  however  a  very  pure  tract,  many 
fibres  of  the  pyramidal  tract  passing  into  it  in  the  pes. 

These  are  the  main  tracts  of  the  pedal  system.  For,  though 
the  nucleus  lenticularis  gives  off  fibres  to  the  internal  capsule, 
our  knowledge  of  the  further  course  of  these  is  at  present 
imperfect,  and  though  there  seem  to  be  longitudinal  fibres 
connecting  the  bulb,  the  pons,  and  the  pes  at  various  levels, 
these  are  not  numerous,  and  at  all  events  do  not  form  con- 
spicuous strands. 


Longitudinal  Fibres  of  the  Tegmental  System. 

§  633.  Cortical  Fibres.  Although  the  fibres  of  the  pedal 
system  form,  as  we  have  seen,  the  greater  part  of,  they  do  not 
form  the  whole  of,  the  internal  capsule.  Fibres  coming  from  all 
or  nearly  all  parts  of  the  cortex  though  they  help  to  form  the 
internal  capsule,  do  not  go  on  to  form  the  pes,  but  pass  to  the  optic 
thalamus  (Fig.  116,  LI.)  and  appear  to  end  in  the  grey  matter  of 
that  body.  In  their  passage  through  the  capsule  the  fibres  of 
this  nature  from  the  frontal  and  parietal  regions  of  the  cortex, 
occupy  the  extreme  front  end  of  the  front  limb  in  front  of  the 
frontal  strand  of  the  fibres  of  the  pedal  system  (Fig.  121,  Th.). 
The  fibres  from  the  occipital  and  temporal  regions,  those  from 
the  occipital  regions  being  the  most  numerous  and  indeed  being 
very  conspicuous,  occupy  the  extreme  hind  end  of  the  hind  limb 
of  the  capsule,  behind  the  temporo-occipital  division  of  the  pedal 
system  (Fig.  121,  Op.).  Since,  as  we  shall  see,  we  have  reason 
to  associate  the  occipital  region  of  the  cortex  with  vision,  the 
fibres  thus  radiating  to  (or  from)  the  thalamus  through  the 
extreme  hind  limb  of  the  capsule  from  (or  to)  the  occipital 
cortex  have  been  called  the  optic  radiation. 

All  the  above  tracts  of  fibres,  though  joining  the  thalamus 
and  not  passing  on  to  the  pes,  take  part  in  the  formation  of  the 
internal  capsule.  But  a  considerable  number  of  fibres  coming 
from  the  temporo-occipital  region  and  especially  from  the  temporal 
region  pass  to  the  thalamus  without  joining  the  capsule ;  they 
pass  ventral  to  and  behind  the  pes  as  this  plunges  into  the 
hemisphere  to  become  the  capsule,  and  so  reach  the  thalamus. 

F.  63 


992         FIBRES   OF   THE   TEGMENTAL   SYSTEM.     [BOOK  in. 

We  may  here  perhaps  diverge  for  a  moment  to  point  out  the 
contrast  between  the  optic  thalamus  and  the  corpus  striatum, 
or  at  least  the  nucleus  caudatus.  The  former  does  not  contribute 
to  the  pedal  system,  the  latter  supplies  a  marked  contribution. 
The  former  receives  fibres  from  all  parts  of  the  cortex ;  there  are 
no  such  special  contributions  from  the  cortex  to  the  latter.  And 
this  difference  accords  with  the  experience  that  when  parts  of 
the  cortex  are  removed,  or  are  congenitally  absent,  no  degenera- 
tion or  want  of  development  is  observed  in  the  corpus  striatum, 
while  degeneration  or  want  of  development  is  observed  in  the 
optic  thalamus  as  well  as  in  parts  of  the  pedal  and  tegmental 
systems.  Hence,  while  we  may  regard  the  optic  thalamus  as  an 
intermediate  mass  of  grey  matter  receiving  fibres  from  the  cortex, 
and  connecting  the  cortex  with  lower  parts  of  the  tegmental 
region,  the  corpus  striatum  appears  rather  to  be  analogous  to  the 
cortex  itself,  to  be  a  special  modification  of  the  cortex,  sending 
fibres  down  into  the  pedal  system,  but  itself  receiving  no  special 
tracts  of  fibres  from  the  cortex.  Indeed  we  may  probably  regard 
the  corpus  striatum  as  the  oldest  part  of  the  superficial  grey 
matter  of  the  hemisphere,  the  more'  ordinary  cortex  being  a  later 
development. 

The  tegmentum  proper,  lying  ventral  to  the  hind  end  of,  and 
behind  the  thalamus,  in  which  region  as  we  have  seen  the  con- 
spicuous red  nucleus  is  situated,  is  thus,  by  reason  of  its  connection 
with  the  thalamus,  indirectly  connected  with  the  cortex.  But 
besides  this,  it  has  direct  connections  of  its  own  with  the  cortex. 
Some  of  the  fibres  of  the  optic  radiation,  as  well  as  fibres  from  the 
temporal  and  occipital  regions  described  above  as  sweeping  round 
the  base  of  the  internal  capsule,  are  said  to  pass  not  to  the  thala- 
mus, but  to  the  tegmentum.  Other  fibres  from  the  frontal  and 
parietal  regions  traversing  the  lenticular  nucleus  in  the  sheets 
•of  white  matter  splitting  the  nucleus  into  parts,  are  also  said 
to  reach  the  tegmentum  either  by  piercing  through  or  by 
sweeping  round  the  internal  capsule.  On  their  path  these  fibres 
are,  according  to  some  observers,  joined  by  fibres  coming  from  the 
lenticular  nucleus  itself,  and  possibly  from  the  caudate  nucleus, 
which  fibres,  on  the  view  that  these  nuclei  are  modified  cortex, 
may  also  be  considered  as  cortical.  Thus  the  forepart  of  the 
tegmental  region  is  brought  into  ample  connection  with  the 
cerebral  hemisphere  partly  by  fibres  joining  the  thalamus,  partly 
by  fibres  passing  directly  to  the  tegmentum  proper. 

The  mode  of  degeneration  of  these  cortical  fibres  of  the 
tegmental  system  is  at  present  a  matter  of  dispute.  Nor  is  the 
general  nature  of  the  fibres  conclusively  determined,  though  it  is 
generally  supposed  that  they  carry  impulses  from  the  thalamus 
and  parts  of  the  tegmentum  to  the  cortex. 

§  634.  In  the  tegmentum  from  the  subthalamic  region  to 
the  bulb  the  reticular  formation  is,  as  we  have  seen,  more  or  less 


CHAP,  ii.]  THE   BRAIN.  993 

abundant ;  this,  and  the  occurrence  of  various  bundles  of  fibres, 
gives  the  region  great  complexity  ;  and  we  must  confine  ourselves 
here  to  touching  on  one  or  two  important  longitudinal  strands 
which  traverse  it. 

The  superior  peduncle  of  the  cerebellum  is  one  of  the  most 
important  of  these.  This  is  on  each  side  a  bundle  of  fibres  which, 
taking  origin  chiefly  from  the  grey  matter  of  the  nucleus  dentatus, 
and  the  smaller  neighbouring  collections  of  grey  matter,  but  also 
in  part  from  the  superficial  grey  matter,  leaves  the  cerebellum  in 
front  of,  and  to  the  median  side  of  the  restiform  body  and  passes 
forward  towards  the  corpora  quadrigemina  to  converge  with  its 
fellow.  At  first  the  two  peduncles  are  superficial  and  dorsal  in 
position  (Figs.  Ill,  112,  8. P.)  and  the  space  between  them  is 
bridged  over  by  the  valve  of  Vieussens  (Fig.  112,  Via);  but,  still 
converging,  they  soon  sink  ventrally  beneath  the  posterior  corpora 
quadrigemina  and  at  the  level  of  the  junction  between  the  anterior 
and  posterior  corpora  quadrigemina  meet  and  decussate  ventral  to 
those  bodies  in  the  ventral  region  of  the  tegmentum  (Fig.  113, 
8.P.).  Beyond  the  decussation  they  are  continued  forwards  in  the 
tegmentum  ventral  to  the  anterior  corpora  quadrigemina  as  two 
strands,  one  on  each  side,  which  appear  to  end  in  the  red  nuclei. 

In  this  way  the  peduncles  connect  certain  parts  of  the  grey 
matter  of  the  cerebellum  with  the  tegmental  region,  and  more 
particularly  with  the  red  nucleus,  and  thus  indirectly  with  the 
structures  with  which  that  region  is  itself  connected. 

The  fillet.  This,  as  we  have  seen  (§  612),  takes  origin  in  the 
bulb,  in  the  interolivary  layer  between  the  inferior  olives,  from 
fibres  which  are  derived  through  the  supra- pyramidal  or  sensory 
decussation  from  the  gracile  and  cuneate  nuclei.  From  this 
origin  it  passes  forward  on  each  side  as  a  flat  band  into  the 
tegmental  region  of  the  pons,  receiving  accessions  from  the  superior 
olive  and  other  collections  of  grey  matter,  and  dividing  there  into 
two  strands,  the  median  (Figs.  112,  113,  Fm)  and  lateral  (Figs. 
112,  113,  Fl  and  Fig.  108,  BF)  fillet.  The  lateral  division  ends 
partly  in  the  grey  matter  of  the  posterior  corpus  quadrigeminum, 
and  partly  in  the  white  matter  underlying  (Fig.  114,  dm)  the 
anterior  corpus  quadrigeminum ;  the  median  division  passing 
farther  forward  appears  partly  to  end  in  the  grey  matter  of  the 
anterior  corpus  quadrigeminum,  but  partly  to  be  continued  on  to 
the  subthalamic  region  of  the  tegmentum  ventral  to  the  thalamus, 
thence  to  the  thalamus,  and  so  to  the  cortex. 

The  longitudinal  posterior  bundles.  In  a  transverse  section 
through  the  fore  part  of  the  pons  at  the  level  of. the  posterior 
corpora  quadrigemina  a  rather  conspicuous  bundle  of  longitudinal 
fibres  (called  the  longitudinal  posterior  bundle)  is  seen  on  each 
side,  cut  transversely,  in  the  dorsal  region  of  the  tegmentum  just 
ventral  to  the  nucleus  of  the  fourth  nerve  (Fig.  113,  I).  Traced 
backward  from  the  aqueduct  beneath  the  fourth  ventricle,  it 

63—2 


994  COMMISSURAL   FIBRES.  [BOOK  in. 

becomes  less  conspicuous  (Fig.  112,  Z)  though  maintaining  its 
position  dorsal  to  the  reticular  formation,  and  at  the  hind  end 
of  the  bulb  appears  to  be  a  continuation  forwards  of  those  fibres, 
"  ground  fibres,"  of  the  anterior  column  of  the  cord  which  probably 
serve  as  successive  short  longitudinal  commissures  between  the 
segments  of  the  cord.  While  the  somewhat  analogous  fillet  runs 
ventral  to  the  reticular  formation,  this  posterior  longitudinal  bundle 
runs  always  dorsal  to  that  structure.  It  may  be  traced  forward  as 
far  as  the  nucleus  of  the  third  nerve,  and  is  seen  in  transverse 
sections  lying  immediately  ventral  to  that  group  of  cells  (Fig. 
114,  l.\  but  its  further  connections  forward  have  not  as  yet  been 
determined.  It  is  relatively  more  prominent  in  the  lower  than 
in  the  higher  animals,  and  its  fibres  acquire  their  medulla 
relatively  early.  It  is  supposed  to  be  connected  with  the  nuclei 
of  the  nerves  governing  the  muscles  of  the  eye,  and  so  to  be 
concerned  in  the  movements  of  that  organ. 

Tracts  from  the  corpora  quadrigemina.  From  each  corpus 
quadrigeminum  there  passes  obliquely  forwards  and  downwards  on 
each  side  a  band  of  fibres,  connected  with  the  grey  matter  of  the 
corpus  and  known  as  its  brachium.  The  anterior  brachium  (Fig. 
114,  Ba),  as  we  shall  see  in  dealing  with  the  optic  nerve,  joins  the 
lateral  corpus  geniculatum  and  helps  to  form  the  optic  tract,  but 
some  of  its  deeper  lying  fibres  proceed  to  the  occipital  cortex 
forming  part  of  the  fibres  which  we  have  (§  633)  described  as 
passing  from  the  occipital  cortex  to  and  past  the  thalamus.  The 
posterior  brachium  passes  to  the  median  corpus  geniculatum ; 
having  received  fibres  from,  and  probably  given  fibres  up  to  that 
body,  it  is  continued  on  to  the  tegmentum,  and  according  to  some 
authors  through  the  tegmentum  by  the  hind  part  of  the  hind  limb 
of  the  internal  capsule  to  the  temporal  region  of  the  cortex, 
mingling  in  its  course  with  fibres  from  the  thalamus. 


Transverse   or  so-called  Commissural  Fibres. 

§  635.  The  two  chief  masses  are  those  on  the  one  hand 
belonging  to  the  cerebrum,  and  those  on  the  other  hand  belonging 
to  the  cerebellum. 

In  the  oerebrum  the  most  imposing  mass  of  transverse  fibres 
forms  the  corpus  callosum.  Starting  from  the  cortex  in  nearly  all 
parts  of  the  hemisphere,  the  fibres  converge  towards  the  thick 
body  of  the  corpus  callosum  placed  in  the  middle  line,  and  thence 
diverge  to  nearly  all  parts  of  the  cortex  of  the  hemisphere  on  the 
other  side,  interlacing  in  their  course  with  the  cortical  fibres  of 
the  pedal  and  tegmental  systems.  It  is  supposed  that  by  means 
of  these  fibres,  each  part  of  the  cortex  of  one  hemisphere  is 
brought  into  connection  with  the  corresponding  part  of  the  other 
hemisphere. 


CHAP,  ii.]  THE   BRAIJST.  995 

Besides  these  callosal  fibres  from  one  hemisphere  to  another, 
the  white  matter  of  each  hemisphere  contains  fibres  called  "  asso- 
ciation fibres,"  passing  from  one  convolution  to  another  of  the 
same  hemisphere. 

The  small  anterior  white  commissure  though  it  is  placed  in  the 
front  part  of  the  third  ventricle  (Fig.  120,  A)  and,  in  part  of  its 
course,  lies  along  the  thalamus  (Fig.  117,  Go)  is  really  a  com- 
missure of  particular  parts  of  the  cerebral  hemispheres.  A 
portion,  very  small  in  man,  belongs  to  the  olfactory  tract;  the 
rest  takes  origin  on  each  side  in  a  limited  portion  of  the  cortex 
(Fig.  116,  Co),  which  we  shall  later  on  speak  of  as  the  temporo- 
sphenoidal  convolution  and  in  which  callosal  fibres  are  deficient, 
whence  it  arches  forward  through  the  globus  pallidus,  past  the 
thalamus  (Figs.  123,  ca,  117,  Co)  to  the  front  part  of  the  third 
ventricle.  It  may  be  remarked  that  this  commissure  is  still  found 
in  those  lower  animals  which  do  not  possess  an  obvious  corpus 
callosum. 

The  small  posterior  commissure  may  be  regarded  as  mainly  a 
commissure  between  the  two  thalami,  but  it  also  helps  to  unite 
the  tegmentum  of  the  two  sides  and  some  fibres  are  said  to  pass 
on  each  side  into  the  hemisphere.  The  middle  or  soft  commissure 
of  the  third  ventricle  (Fig.  115,  c),  though  it  contains  transverse 
fibres,  is  in  the  main  a  collection  of  grey  matter,  indeed  a  part  of 
the  central  grey  matter. 

The  fornix,  together  with,  at  all  events,  part  of  the  septum 
lucidum  which  joins  it  with  the  corpus  callosum,  must  also  be 
regarded  as  a  commissural  structure.  But  its  relations  are 
peculiar ;  for  while,  behind,  the  diverging  posterior  pillars  begin  in 
the  cerebral  hemispheres,  namely,  in  the  walls  of  the  descending 
horn  of  the  lateral  ventricle  on  each  side,  in  front  the  anterior 
pillars  or  columns,  leaving  the  cerebral  hemispheres,  pass  along 
the  lateral  walls  of  the  third  ventricle  (Fig.  120,  /),  and 
apparently  end  in  the  grey  matter  of  the  corpora  albicantia. 
Whether  the  band  of  fibres,  known  as  Vicq  d'Azyr's  bundle  (Fig. 
116,  FZ>),  which  running  in  the  lateral  wall  of  the  third  ventricle 
leads  dorsally  from  each  corpus  albicans  up  to  the  anterior  nucleus 
of  the  thalamus,  is  really  to  be  considered  as  a  continuation  of  the 
fornix  is  disputed ;  it  may  more  probably  be  regarded  as  a  part  of 
the  system  spoken  of  above  as  connecting  the  cortex  with  the 
thalamus. 

In  the  cerebellum  true  commissural  fibres,  are  supplied  by  the 
middle  peduncles ;  but  by  no  means  all  the  fibres  of  these  peduncles 
are  of  this  nature.  The  fibres  of  the  middle  peduncle,  in  contrast 
to  those  of  the  superior  peduncle  which  start  chiefly  from  the 
nucleus  dentatus,  or  other  internal  grey  matter,  and  to  those  of 
the  inferior  peduncle  which  start  chiefly  from  the  superficial  grey 
matter  of  the  vermis,  appear  to  start  from  the  superficial  grey 
matter  of  the  whole  surface,  from  that  of  the  median  vermis  as 


996  COMMISSURAL   FIBRES.  [BOOK  in. 

well  as  from  that  of  the  lateral  hemispheres ;  they  thus  form  the 
greater  part  of  the  central  white  matter.  Sweeping  down  into 
the  pons,  they  form  the  transverse  fibres  of  that  body,  interlacing 
with  the  longitudinal  fibres  of  the  crural  system  and  intermingling 
with  the  abundant  grey  matter. 

Of  these  transverse  fibres  of  the  pons,  a  certain  number  are 
truly  commissural;  they  make  no  connections  with  cells  in  the 
pons,  but  continue  their  way  unbroken  across  it ;  they  start  in  the 
superficial  grey  matter  of  one  side  of  the  cerebellum  and  end  in 
the  superficial  grey  matter  of  the  other  side,  the  parts  of  the  grey 
matter  thus  united  being  probably  corresponding  parts.  The 
most  ventrally  placed  transverse  fibres  of  the  pons,  which  form  a 
superficial  layer  of  white  matter,  free  from  grey  matter  (Fig.  Ill, 
tr.  P.)  are  probably  of  this  nature,  as  are  also  the  transverse  fibres 
placed  most  dorsally,  just  ventral  to  the  tegmental  region. 

A  large  number  of  the  transverse  fibres  are  not  of  this  nature. 
They  cross  from  one  side  of  the  cerebellum  to  the  opposite  side  of 
the  pons,  but  end  in  the  pons  apparently  in  the  nerve  cells  of  the 
grey  matter ;  and  it  is  supposed,  that  by  these  nerve  cells  they 
are  brought  into  connection  with  the  longitudinal  fibres  of  the 
pedal  system  and  thus  with  the  cerebrum.  They  are  transverse 
appendages  of  the  pedal  system,  not  true  commissural  fibres 
though  they  do  cross  the  median  line. 

It  is  further  supposed  that  other  fibres  of  the  middle  peduncle 
reaching  the  pons  do  not  cross  the  median  line,  but  keeping  to 
the  same  side  and  changing  their  direction,  take  a  longitudinal 
upward  course  either  with  or  without  the  intervention  of  nerve 
cells,  and  so  make  their  way  to  the  tegmentum.  But  this  is  not 
certain. 

We  must  also  consider  as  commissural  structures  the  numerous 
fibres  crossing,  or  serving  to  form  the  median  raphe  in  the  bulb. 
This  raphe,  with  similar  commissural  fibres,  is  present  in  the  teg- 
mental  portion  of  the  pons,  and  indeed  in  the  tegmentum  itself. 

Fibres  also  cross  from  one  side  to  the  other  in  connection  with 
the  cranial  nerves,  but  these  as  well  as  all  the  tracts  specially 
connected  with  the  cranial  nerves,  including  the  olfactory  and 
optic  nerves,  had  better  be  considered  by  themselves. 

Summary, 

§  636.  It  may  perhaps  appear  from  the  foregoing  that  the 
brain  consists  of  a  number  of  isolated  masses  of  grey  matter,  some 
large,  some  small,  connected  together  by  a  multitude  of  ties  of 
white  matter  arranged  in  perplexing  intricacy ;  and  the  addition 
of  numerous  collections  of  grey  matter  and  strands  of  white  matter 
of  which  we  have  made  no  mention  would  still  further  increase  the 
perplexity.  Nevertheless  a  systematic  arrangement  may  be  recog- 
nized, at  least  to  a  certain  extent. 


CHAP,  ii.]  THE   BRAIN.  997 

The  least  conspicuous,  but  perhaps  in  point  of  origin  the  oldest 
part  of  the  brain,  seems  to  be  what  we  have  called  the  central  grey 
matter.  This  seems  to  serve  chiefly  as  a  bed  for  the  development 
of  the  nuclei  of  the  cranial  nerves. 

Next  to  the  central  grey  matter  and  more  or  less  associated 
with  it  comes  what  we  have  called  the  tegmental  region,  of  which 
the  reticular  formation,  coming  into  prominence  in  the  bulb  and 
continued  on  to  the  subthalamic  region,  forms  as  it  were  the  core. 
Belonging  to  the  tegmental  system  are  numerous  masses  of  grey 
matter  from  the  conspicuous  optic  thalamus  and  the  red  nucleus 
in  front  to  the  several  nuclei  of  the  bulb  behind.  This  complex 
tegmental  system,  which  may  perhaps  be  regarded  as  a  more  or 
less  continuous  column  of  grey  matter,  comparable  to  the  grey 
matter  of  the  spinal  cord,  serves  as  a  sort  of  back  bone  to  the  rest 
of  the  central  nervous  system.  With  the  spinal  cord  it  is  con- 
nected by  various  ties,  besides  being  as  it  were  a  continuation  of 
the  spinal  grey  matter,  and  around  it  are  builc  up  the  great 
mass  of  the  cerebrum,  and  the  smaller  but  still  large  mass  of  the 
cerebellum ;  the  less  important  corpora  quadrigemina  we  may  for 
simplicity's  sake  neglect. 

At  the  hind  end  we  find  various  parts  of  the  spinal  cord 
becoming  connected  with  this  tegmental  system,  either  passing 
into  it  and  becoming,  as  far  as  our  present  knowledge  goes,  lost  in 
it,  or  supplying  strands  or  fibres  which  passing  into  it  become 
through  it  connected  with  other  parts.  Thus  the  anterior  column 
of  the  cord  exclusive  of  the  direct  pyramidal  tract,  the  lateral 
column  exclusive  of  the  crossed  pyramidal  and  cerebellar  tracts 
(and  possibly  the  antero-lateral  ascending  tract),  together  with 
part  of  the  posterior  column  appear  to  join  the  tegmental  system, 
while  part  of  the  posterior  column,  after  the  relay  of  the  gracile 
and  cuneate  nuclei,  passes  through  the  system  as  the  fillet  destined 
for  various  structures. 

At  the  front  end  we  find  all  parts  of  the  cerebral  cortex 
(though  some  regions,  namely  the  temporo-occipital,  to  a  greater 
extent  than  others),  connected  with  the  thalamus  and  other  parts 
of  the  tegmental  system;  and,  as  we  have  seen,  the  corpus 
striatum  may  possibly  possess  like  connections. 

The  relations  of  the  cerebellum  to  this  system  are  notable.  On 
the  one  hand  the  cerebellum  is  directly  connected  with  the  system, 
partly  by  fibres  which  pass  from  the  bulb  to  join  the  restiform 
body  or  inferior  peduncle,  partly  by  the  superior  peduncles  which, 
as  we  have  seen,  are  in  a  measure  lost  in  the  tegmenfcum,  and  partly 
probably  by  fibres  of  the  middle  peduncles  also  making  connections 
with  the  tegmentum.  On  the  other  hand  the  cerebellum  forms 
around  the  tegmental  system  a  great  junction  between  the  spinal 
cord  and  the  cerebrum.  To  the  spinal  cord  it  is  joined  in  a 
direct  manner  by  the  cerebellar  tract  and  possibly  by  the  antero- 
lateral  ascending  tract,  and  in  an  indirect  manner  by  the  relay 


998  STRUCTURE   OF   THE   BRAIN.  [BOOK  m. 

of  the  gracile  and  cuneate  nuclei.  To  all  parts  of  the  cerebral 
cortex,  it  appears  to  be  joined  by  those  conspicuous  strands  of  the 
pedal  system,  which,  as  we  have  seen,  end  in  the  pons,  and  there 
make  connections  with  the  fibres  of  the  middle  peduncle.  And 
we  may  here  perhaps  remark  that  while  this  connection  between 
the  cerebrum  and  cerebellum  is  wholly  a  crossed  one,  each  cerebral 
hemisphere  being  joined  with  the  opposite  half  of  the  cerebellum, 
the  connections  between  the  spinal  cord  and  the  cerebellum  are 
largely  uncrossed  ones,  that  by  the  cerebellar  tract  being  wholly 
uncrossed,  and  that  with  the  posterior  column  by  the  relay  of  the 
gracile  and  cuneate  nuclei  being  in  part  uncrossed. 

Thus  the  cerebral  cortex  has  a  double  hold,  so  to  speak,  on  the 
rest  of  the  central  nervous  system  first  through  the  tegmental 
system,  and  secondly  through  the  cerebellar  junction.  But  in 
addition  to  this  there  is  another  tie  between  the  cerebral  cortex 
and  the  whole  length  of  the  cerebro-spinal  axis,  or  at  least  between 
it  and  the  whole  series  of  motor  mechanisms  in  succession  from 
the  nucleus  of  the  third  nerve  to  the  nucleus,  if  we  may  so  call 
it,  of  the  anterior  root  of  the  coccygeal  nerve,  namely,  the  great 
pyramidal  tract,  which  thus  appears  as  a  something  superadded 
to  all  the  rest  of  the  central  nervous  system. 

When  the  cerebral  hemispheres  are  removed  this  pyramidal 
tract  falls  away  as  does  also  the  pedal  system  leading  from  the 
cerebrum  to  the  pons,  but  there  still  remains  the  tegmental  system 
with  its  cerebellar  and  other  adjuncts  and  this,  as  we  shall  see, 
constitutes  a  nervous  machinery,  capable  of  carrying  out  exceed- 
ingly complicated  acts. 


SEC.   4.     ON  THE  PHENOMENA   EXHIBITED   BY   AN 
ANIMAL   DEPRIVED   OF  ITS  CEREBRAL  HEMISPHERES. 


§  637.  The  cerebral  hemispheres,  as  we  have  more  than  once 
insisted,  seem  to  stand  apart  from  the  rest  of  the  brain.  In  the 
case  of  some  animals  it  is  possible  to  remove  the  cerebral  hemi- 
spheres and  to  keep  the  animal  not  only  alive,  but  in  good  health 
for  a  long  time,  days,  weeks,  or  even  months  after  the  operation. 
In  such  case  we  are  able  to  study  the  behaviour  of  an  animal 
possessing  no  cerebral  hemispheres  and  to  compare  it  with  that  of 
an  intact  animal.  Such  an  experiment  is  best  carried  out  on  a 
frog.  In  this  animal  it  is  comparatively  easy  to  remove  the 
cerebral  hemispheres,  including  the  parts  corresponding  to  the 
corpora  striata,  leaving  behind  intact  and  uninjured  the  optic 
thalami  with  the  optic  nerves,  the  optic  lobes  (or  representatives 
of  the  corpora  quadrigemina),  the  small  cerebellum  and  the  bulb. 
If  the  animal  be  carefully  fed  and  attended  to,  it  may  be  kept 
alive  for  a  very  long  time,  for  more  than  a  year  for  instance. 

The  salient  fact  about  a  frog  lacking  the  cerebral  hemispheres, 
is  that,  as  in  the  case  of  a  frog  deprived  of  its  whole  brain,  the 
signs  of  the  working  of  an  intelligent  volition  are  either  wholly 
absent  or  extremely  rare.  The  presence  of  the  bulb  and  the 
middle  parts  of  the  brain  (for  so  we  may  conveniently  call  the 
cerebral  structures  lying  between  the  cerebral  hemispheres  and 
the  bulb)  ensures  the  healthy  action  of  the  vascular,  respiratory 
and  other  nutritive  systems ;  food  placed  in  the  mouth  is  readily 
and  easily  swallowed ;  the  animal  when  stimulated  executes  various 
movements ;  but  if  it  be  left  entirely  to  itself,  and  care  be  taken  to 
shield  it  from  adventitious  stimuli,  either  it  remains  perfectly  and 
permanently  quiescent,  or  the  apparently  spontaneous  movements 
which  it  carries  out  are  so  few  and  so  limited  as  to  make  it  very 
doubtful  whether  they  can  fairly  be  called  volitional.  Such  a  frog, 
for  instance,  after  being  kept  alive  for  some  time  and  made  to 
exhibit  the  phenomena  of  which  we  are  about  to  speak,  has  been 
placed  on  a  table  with  a  line  drawn  in  chalk  around  the  area 
covered  by  its  body,  and  left  to  itself  has  subsequently  been  found 
dead  without  having  stirred  outside  the  chalked  circle. 


1000         WITHOUT   CEREBRAL   HEMISPHERES.     [BOOK  in. 

We  must  here  however  repeat  the  caution  laid  down  in 
§  582,  as  to  the  ultimate  effects  of  an  operation  on  the  central 
nervous  system.  The  longer  the  frog  is  kept  alive  and  in  good 
health  after  the  removal  of  the  cerebral  hemispheres,  the  greater 
is  the  tendency  for  apparently  spontaneous  movements  to  shew 
themselves.  For  days  or  even  weeks  after  the  operation  there 
may  be  no  signs  whatever  of  the  working  of  any  volition ;  but 
after  the  lapse  of  months,  movements,  previously  absent,  of  such 
a  character  as  to  suggest  that  they  ought  to  be  called  voluntary, 
may  make  their  appearance.  To  this  point  we  shall  return,  but 
may  in  the  meanwhile  state  that  even  in  their  most  complete 
development  such  movements  do  not  negative  the  view  that  the 
frog  in  the  absence  of  the  cerebral  hemispheres  is  wanting  in 
what  we  ordinarily  call  a  'will.' 

§  638.  We  have  seen  that  a  frog  from  which  the  whole  brain 
has  been  removed  and  the  spinal  cord  only  left  appears  similarly 
devoid  of  a  '  will ;'  but  the  phenomena  presented  by  a  frog 
possessing  the  middle  portions  of  the  brain  differ  widely  from 
those  presented  by  a  frog  possessing  a  spinal  cord  only.  We  may 
perhaps  broadly  describe  the  behaviour  of  a  frog  from  which  the 
cerebral  hemispheres  only  have  been  removed,  by  saying  that  such 
an  animal,  though  exhibiting  no  spontaneous  movements,  can  by 
the  application  of  appropriate  stimuli  be  induced  to  perform  all 
or  nearly  all  the  movements  which  an  entire  frog  is  capable  of 
executing.  It  can  be  made  to  swim,  to  leap,  and  to  crawl.  Left 
to  itself  it  assumes  what  may  be  called  the  natural  posture  of  a 
frog,  with  the  fore  limbs  erect,  and  the  hind  limbs  flexed,  so  that 
the  line  of  the  body  makes  an  angle  with  the  surface  on  which  it 
is  resting.  When  placed  on  its  back,  it  immediately  regains  this 
natural  posture.  When  placed  on  a  board,  it  does  not  fall  from  the 
board  when  the  latter  is  tilted  up  so  as  to  displace  the  animal's  centre 
of  gravity :  it  crawls  up  the  board  until  it  gains  a  new  position 
in  which  its  centre  of  gravity  is  restored  to  its  proper  place.  Its 
movements  are  exactly  those  of  an  entire  frog  except  that  they 
need  an  external  stimulus  to  call  them  forth.  They  differ  moreover 
fundamentally  from  those  of  an  entire  frog  in  the  following  impor- 
tant feature ;  they  inevitably  follow  when  the  stimulus  is  applied ; 
they  come  to  an  end  when  the  stimulus  ceases  to  act.  By 
continually  varying  the  inclination  of  a  board  on  which  it  is  placed, 
the  frog  may  be  made  to  continue  crawling  almost  indefinitely ; 
but  directly  the  board  is  made  to  assume  such  a  position  that  the 
body  of  the  frog  is  in  equilibrium,  the  crawling  ceases ;  and  if  the 
position  be  not  disturbed  the  animal  will  remain  impassive  and 
quiet  for  an  almost  indefinite  time.  When  thrown  into  water,  the 
creature  begins  at  once  to  swim  about  in  the  most  regular  manner, 
and  will  continue  to  swim  until  it  is  exhausted,  if  there  be  nothing 
present  on  which  it  can  come  to  rest.  If  a  small  piece  of  wood  be 
placed  on  the  water  the  frog  will,  when  it  comes  in  contact  with 


CHAP,  ii.]  THE   BRAIN.  1001 

the  wood,  crawl  upon  it,  and  so  come  to  rest.  If  disturbed  from 
its  natural  posture,  as  by  being  placed  on  its  back,  it  immediately 
struggles  to  regain  that  posture;  only  by  the  application  of 
continued  force  can  it  be  kept  lying  on  its  back.  Such  a  frog,  if 
its  flanks  be  gently  stroked,  will  croak ;  and  the  croaks  follow  so 
regularly  and  surely  upon  the  strokes  that  the  animal  may  almost 
be  played  upon  like  a  musical,  or  at  least  an  acoustic  instrument. 
Moreover,  provided  that  the  optic  nerves  and  their  arrangements 
have  not  been  injured  by  the  operation,  the  movements  of  the 
animal  appear  to  be  influenced  by  light ;  if  it  be  urged  to  move 
in  any  particular  direction,  it  seems  in  its  progress  to  avoid 
obstacles,  at  least  such  as  cast  a  strong  shadow ;  it  turns  its  course 
to  the  right  or  left  or  sometimes  leaps  over  the  obstacle.  In  fact, 
even  to  a  careful  observer  the  differences  between  such  a  frog  and 
an  entire  frog  which  was  simply  very  stupid  or  very  inert,  would 
appear  slight  and  unimportant  except  in  this,  that  the  animal 
without  its  cerebral  hemispheres  is  obedient  to  every  stimulus, 
and  that  each  stimulus  evokes  an  appropriate  movement,  whereas 
with  the  entire  animal  it  is  impossible  to  predict  whether  any 
result  at  all,  and  if  so  what  result,  will  follow  the  application  of 
this  or  that  stimulus.  Both  may  be  regarded  as  machines ;  but 
the  one  is  a  machine  and  nothing  more,  the  other  is  a  machine 
governed  and  checked  by  a  dominant  volition. 

Now  such  movements  as  crawling,  leaping,  swimming,  and 
indeed,  as  we  have  already  urged,  to  a  greater  or  less  extent, 
all  bodily  movements,  are  carried  out  by  means  of  coordinate 
nervous  motor  impulses,  influenced,  arranged,  and  governed  by 
coincident  sensory  or  afferent  impulses.  Muscular  movements 
are  determined  by  afferent  influences  proceeding  from  the  muscles 
and  constituting  the  foundation  of  the  muscular  sense ;  they  are 
also  directed  by  means  of  afferent  impulses  passing  centripetally 
along  the  sensory  nerves  of  the  skin,  the  eye,  the  ear,  and  other 
organs.  Independently  of  the  particular  afferent  impulses,  which 
acting  as  a  stimulus  call  forth  the  movement,  very  many  other 
afferent  impulses  are  concerned  in  the  generation  and  coordination 
of  the  resultant  motor  impulses.  Every  bodily  movement  such 
as  those  of  which  we  are  speaking  is  the  work  of  a  more  or  less 
complicated  nervous  mechanism,  in  which  there  are  not  only 
central  and  efferent,  but  also  afferent  factors.  And,  putting 
aside  the  question  of  consciousness,  with  which  we  have  here 
no  occasion  to  deal,  it  is  evident  that  in  the  frog  deprived  of 
its  cerebral  hemispheres  all  these  factors  are  present,  the  afferent 
no  less  than  the  central  and  the  efferent.  The  machinery  for  all 
the  necessary  and  usual  bodily  movements  is  present  in  all  its 
completeness.  We  may  regard  the  share  therefore  which  the 
cerebral  hemispheres  take  in  executing  the  movements  of  which 
the  entire  animal  is  capable,  as  that  of  putting  this  machinery 
into  action  or  of  limiting  its  previous  activity.  The  relation 


1002         WITHOUT   CEREBRAL   HEMISPHERES.      [BOOK  m. 

which  the  higher  nervous  changes  concerned  in  volition  bear  to 
this  machinery  may  be  compared  to  that  of  a  stimulus,  always 
bearing  in  mind  that  the  effect  of  a  stimulus  on  a  nervous  centre 
may  be  either  to  start  activity,  or  to  increase,  or  to  curb,  or  to 
stop  activity  already  present.  We  might  almost  speak  of  the 
will  as  an  intrinsic  stimulus.  Its  operations  are  limited  by  the 
machinery  at  its  command.  We  may  infer  that  in  the  frog, 
the  action  of  the  cerebral  hemispheres  in  giving  shape  to  a 
bodily  movement  is  that  of  throwing  into  activity  particular- 
parts  of  the  nervous  machinery  situated  in  the  lower  parts  of 
the  brain  and  in  the  spinal  cord ;  precisely  the  same  movement 
may  be  initiated  in  the  absence  of  the  cerebral  hemispheres 
by  applying  such  stimuli  as  shall  throw  precisely  the  same 
parts  of  that  machinery  into  the  same  activity. 

Very  marked  is  the  contrast  between  the  behaviour  of  such 
a  frog  which,  though  deprived  of  its  cerebral  hemispheres,  still 
retains  the  other  parts  of  the  brain,  and  that  of  a  frog  which 
possesses  a  spinal  cord  only.  The  latter  when  placed  on  its 
back  makes  no  attempt  to  regain  its  normal  posture ;  in  fact, 
it  may  be  said  to  have  completely  lost  its  normal  posture,  for 
even  when  placed  on  its  belly  it  does  not  stand  with  its  fore 
feet  erect,  as  does  the  other  animal,  but  lies  flat  on  the  ground. 
When  thrown  into  water,  instead  of  swimming,  it  sinks  like  a 
lump  of  lead.  When  pinched,  or  otherwise  stimulated,  it  does 
not  crawl  or  leap  forwards;  it  simply  throws  out  its  limbs  in 
various  ways.  When  its  flanks  are  stroked  it  does  not  croak ; 
and  when  a  board  on  which  it  is  placed  is  inclined  sufficiently 
to  displace  its  centre  of  gravity  it  makes  no  effort  to  regain 
its  balance,  but  falls  off  the  board  like  a  lifeless  mass.  Though, 
as  we  have  seen,  the  various  parts  of  the  spinal  cord  of  the  frog 
contain  a  large  amount  of  coordinating  machinery,  so  that  the 
brainless  frog  may,  by  appropriate  stimuli,  be  made  to  execute 
various  purposeful  coordinate  movements,  yet  these  are  very 
limited  compared  with  those  which  can  be  similarly  carried 
out  by  a  frog  possessing  the  middle  and  lower  parts  of  the 
brain  in  addition  to  the  spinal  cord.  It  is  evident  that  a  great 
deal  of  the  more  complex  machinery  of  this  kind,  especially  all 
that  which  has  to  deal  with  the  body  as  a  whole,  and  all  that 
which  is  concerned  with  equilibrium  and  is  specially  governed 
by  the  higher  senses,  is  seated  not  in  the  spinal  cord  but  in 
the  brain.  We  do  not  wish  now  to  discuss  the  details  of  this 
machinery ;  all  we  desire  to  insist  upon  at  present  is  that,  in 
the  frog  the  nervous  machinery  required  for  the  execution,  as 
distinguished  from  the  origination,  of  bodily  movements  even 
of  the  most  complicated  kind,  is  present  after  complete  removal 
of  the  cerebral  hemispheres,  though  these  movements  are  such 
as  to  require  the  cooperation  of  highly  differentiated  afferent 
impulses. 


CHAP,  ii.]  THE   BRAIK  1003 

§  639.  In  warm-blooded  animals  the  removal  of  the  cerebral 
hemispheres  is  attended  with  much  greater  difficulties  than  in  the 
case  of  the  frog.  Nevertheless,  in  the  bird  the  operation  may  be 
carried  out  with  approximate  success.-  Pigeons  for  instance  have 
been  kept  alive  for  five  or  six  weeks  after  complete  removal  of  the 
cerebral  hemispheres,  with  the  exception  of  portions  of  the  crura 
and  corpora  striata  immediately  surrounding  the  optic  thalami ; 
these  parts  were  left  in  order  to  ensure  the  intact  condition  of 
the  latter  bodies. 

When  the  immediate  effects  of  the  operation  have  passed  off, 
and  for  some  time  afterwards,  the  appearance  and  behaviour  of 
the  bird  are  strikingly  similar  to  those  of  a  bird  exceedingly  sleepy 
and  stupid.  It  is  able  to  maintain  what  appears  to  be  a  completely 
normal  posture,  and  can  balance  itself  on  one  leg,  after  the  fashion 
of  a  bird  which  has  in  a  natural  way  gone  to  sleep.  Left  alone  in 
perfect  quiet,  it  will  remain  impassive  and  motionless  for  a  long 
time.  When  stirred  it  moves,  shifts  its  position;  and  then,  on 
being  left  alone,  returns  to  a  natural,  easy  posture.  Placed  on 
its  side  or  its  back  it  will  regain  its  feet;  thrown  into  the  air, 
it  flies  with  considerable  precision  for  some  distance  before  it 
returns  to  rest.  It  frequently  tucks  its  head  under  its  wings, 
and  at  times  may  be  seen  to  clean  its  feathers ;  when  its  beak 
is  plunged  into  corn,  it  eats.  It  may  be  induced  to  move  not 
only  by  ordinary  stimuli  applied  to  the  skin,  but  also  by  sudden 
loud  sounds,  or  by  flashes  of  light;  in  its  flight  it  will,  though 
imperfectly,  avoid  obstacles,  and  its  various  movements  appear 
to  be  to  a  certain  extent  guided  not  only  by  touch  but  also  by 
visual  impressions. 

In  a  certain  number  of  cases  this  sleepy,  drowsy  condition 
passes  off  and  is  succeeded  by  a  phase  in  which  the  bird,  apparently 
spontaneously,  without  the  intervention  of  any  obvious  stimulus, 
moves  rapidly  about.  It  does  not  fly,  that  is  to  say,  it  does  not 
raise  itself  from  the  ground  in  flight,  but  walks  about  incessantly 
for  a  long  while  at  a  time,  periods  of  activity  alternating  with 
periods  of  repose.  It  seems,  from  time  to  time,  to  wake  up  and 
move  about,  and  then  to  go  to  sleep  again;  and  it  has  been 
observed  that  during  the  night  it  appears  to  be  always  asleep. 
It  is  obvious,  therefore,  that  the  sleepy,  quiescent  condition  is 
not  due  simply  to  the  absence  of  the  cerebral  hemispheres,  but 
is  a  temporary  effect  of  the  operation,  and  that  spontaneous 
movements,  that  is  to  say,  movements  not  started  by  any  obvious 
stimulus,  may  occur  after  removal  of  the  cerebral  hemispheres. 
But  the  movements  so  witnessed  differ  from  those  of  an  intact 
bird.  They  are,  it  is  true,  varied ;  and  the  variations  are  in  part 
dependent  on  external  circumstances,  the  bird  being  guided  by 
tactile,  and,  as  we  have  said,  visual  sensations,  or,  to  be  more 
exact,  by  impressions  made  upon  the  sensory  nerves  of  the  skin 
and  on  the  retina;  but  they  do  not  shew  the  wide  variations  of 


1004         WITHOUT   CEREBRAL   HEMISPHERES.     [Boon  in. 

voluntary  movements.  The  bird  never  flies  up  from  the  ground, 
never  spontaneously  picks  up  corn,  and  its  aimless,  monotonous, 
restless  walks,  resembling  the  continued  swimming  of  the  frog 
thrown  into  the  water  after  being  deprived  of  its  cerebral 
hemispheres,  forcibly  suggest  that  the  activity  is  the  outcome 
of  some  intrinsic  impulse  generated  in  the  nervous  machinery 
in  some  way  or  other,  but  not  by  the  working  of  a  conscious 
intelligence  as  in  the  impulse  which  we  call  the  will. 

Still  we  must  not  shut  our  eyes  to  the  fact  that  spontaneous 
movements,  whatever  their  exact  nature,  are  manifested  by  a  bird 
in  the  absence  of  the  cerebral  hemispheres,  and  become  the  more 
striking  the  more  complete  the  recovery  from  the  passing  effects 
of  the  mere  operation.  Could  such  birds  be  kept  alive  for  any 
considerable  time,  possibly  further  developments  might  be  wit- 
nessed, and  indeed  cases  are  on  record  where  birds  have  been 
kept  alive  for  months  after  the  operation,  and  have  shewn  sponta- 
neous movements  of  a  still  more  varied  character  than  those  just 
described ;  but  in  such  cases  the  removal  of  the  hemispheres  has 
not  been  complete,  portions  of  the  ventral  regions  being  left 
behind;  and,  though  a  mere  remnant  left  around  the  optic  thalami 
can  hardly  be  regarded  as  a  sufficient  cause  for  the  spontaneity  of 
which  we  are  speaking,  a  larger  mass,  still  more  or  less  retaining 
its  normal  structure,  might  have  a  marked  effect.  And  we  may 
here  perhaps  remark  that  all  these  facts  seem  to  point  to  the 
conclusion  that  what  may  be  called  mechanical  spontaneity, 
sometimes  spoken  of  as  'automatism,'  differs  from  the  sponta- 
neity of  the  'will'  in  degree  rather  than  in  kind.  Looking  at 
the  matter  from  a  purely  physiological  point  of  view  (the  only 
one  which  has  a  right  to  be  employed  in  these  pages),  the  real 
difference  between  an  automatic  act  and  a  voluntary  act  is  that 
the  chain  of  physiological  events  between  the  act  and  its  physio- 
logical cause  is  in  the  one  case  short  and  simple,  in  the  other  long 
and  complex.  We  have  seen  that  a  frog  lacking  its  cerebral 
hemispheres,  viewed  from  one  stand  point,  appears  in  the  light  of 
a  mechanical  apparatus,  on  which  each  change  of  circumstances 
produces  a  direct,  unvarying,  inevitable  effect.  And  yet  it  is  on 
record  that  such  a  frog,  if  kept  alive  long  enough  for  the  most 
complete  disappearance  of  the  direct  effects  of  the  operation,  will 
bury  itself  in  the  earth  at  the  approach  of  winter,  and  is  able  to 
catch  and  swallow  flies  and  other  food  coming  in  its  neighbourhood, 
although  in  other  respects  it  shews  no  signs  of  an  intelligent 
volition,  and  answers  with  unerring  mechanical  certainty  to  the 
play  of  stimuli.  We  may  add  that  in  some  fishes  the  removal  of 
their  cerebral  hemispheres,  which  in  these  animals  form  a  relatively 
small  part  of  the  whole  brain,  produces  exceedingly  little  change 
in  their  general  behaviour. 

These  however  are  not  the  considerations  on  which  we  wish 
here  to  dwell ;  we  have  quoted  the  behaviour  of  the  bird  deprived 


CHAP,  ii.]  THE   BRAIN.  1005 

of  its  cerebral  hemisphere  mainly  to  shew  that  in  this  warm- 
blooded animal,  as  in  the  more  lowly  cold-blooded  frog,  the  parts 
of  the  brain  below  or  behind  the  cerebral  hemispheres  constitute 
a  nervous  machinery  by  which  all  the  ordinary  bodily  movements 
may  be  carried  out.  The  bird,  like  the  frog,  suffers  no  paralysis 
when  the  cerebral  hemispheres  are  removed ;  on  the  contrary, 
though  its  movements  have  not  been  studied  so  closely  as  those  of 
the  frog,  the  bird  without  its  cerebral  hemispheres  seems  capable 
of  executing  at  all  events  all  the  ordinary  bodily  movements  of  a 
bird.  And  in  the  bird  as  in  the  frog,  the  afferent  impulses 
passing  into  the  central  nervous  system,  whether  they  give  rise  to 
consciousness  or  no,  play  an  important  part  not  only  in  originating 
but  in  guiding  and  coordinating  the  efferent  impulses  which  stir 
the  muscles  to  contract,  the  coordination  being  effected  partly  in 
the  spinal  cord,  but  largely  and  indeed  chiefly  in  the  parts  of  the 
brain  lying  behind  the  cerebral  hemispheres.  It  is  further  worthy 
of  notice  that  spontaneity  of  movement  of  the  kind  which  we  have 
described,  is  much  more  prominent  in  the  more  highly  developed 
bird,  than  in  the  more  lowly  frog.  The  cerebral  hemispheres  are 
not  the  only  part  of  the  central  nervous  system  which  has  under- 
gone a  greater  development  in  the  bird ;  the  other  parts  of  the 
brain  have  also  acquired  a  far  greater  complexity  than  in  the  frog. 
§  640.  In  the  mammal  the  removal  of  the  cerebral  hemi- 
spheres is  still  more  difficult  than  in  the  bird ;  the  animal  cannot 
be  kept  alive  for  more  than  a  few  hours ;  but  in  some  mammals  it 
is  possible  to  observe  during  those  few  hours  phenomena  kindred 
to  those  witnessed  in  the  bird  and  in  the  frog.  The  rabbit  or  rat, 
from  which  the  whole  of  both  hemispheres  has  been  removed 
with  the  exception  of  the  parts  immediately  surrounding  the  optic 
thalami,  can  stand,  run  and  leap.  Placed  on  its  side  or  back  it  at 
once  regains  its  feet.  Left  alone  it  generally  remains  as  motion- 
less and  impassive  as  a  statue,  save  now  and  then  when  a  passing 
impulse  seems  to  stir  it  to  a  sudden  but  brief  movement ;  but 
sometimes  it  seems  subject  to  a  more  continued  impulse  to  move, 
in  which  case  death  usually  follows  very  speedily.  Such  a  rabbit 
will  remain  for  minutes  together  utterly  heedless  of  a  carrot  or 
cabbage-leaf  placed  just  before  its  nose,  though  if  a  morsel  be 
placed  within  its  mouth  it  at  once  begins  to  eat.  When  stirred  it 
will  with  ease  and  steadiness  run  or  leap  forward ;  and  obstacles 
in  its  course  are  very  frequently,  with  more  or  less  success,  avoided. 
In  some  cases  the  animal  (rat)  has  been  described  as  following  by 
movements  of  the  head  a  bright  light  held  in  front  of  it  (provided 
that  the  optic  nerves  and  tracts  have  not  been  injured  during  the 
operation),  as  starting  when  a  shrill  and  loud  noise  is  made  near 
it,  and  as  crying  when  pinched,  often  with  a  long  and  seemingly 
plaintive  scream.  So  plaintive  is  the  cry  which  it  thus  gives 
forth  as  to  suggest  to  the  observer  the  existence  of  passion,  this, 
however,  is  probably  a  wrong  interpretation  of  a  vocal  action ; 


1006         WITHOUT   CEREBRAL   HEMISPHERES.     [BOOK  in. 

the  cry  appears  plaintive  simply  because,  in  consequence  of  the 
completeness  of  the  reflex  nervous  machinery  and  the  absence  of 
the  usual  restraints,  it  is  prolonged. 

Without  insisting  too  much  on  such  results  as  these,  and 
allowing  full  weight  to  the  objection  which  may  be  urged,  that  in 
some  of  these  cases  parts  of  the  cerebral  hemispheres  surrounding 
the  optic  thalami  were  left,  there  still  remains  adequate  evidence 
to  shew  that  a  mammal  such  as  a  rabbit,  in  the  same  way  as 
a  frog  and  a  bird,  may  in  the  complete  or  all  but  complete 
absence  of  the  cerebral  hemispheres  maintain  a  natural  posture, 
free  from  all  signs  of  disturbance  of  equilibrium,  and  is  able  to 
carry  out  with  success,  at  all  events  all  the  usual  and  common 
bodily  movements.  And  as  in  the  bird  and  frog,  the  evidence 
also  shews  that  these  movements  not  only  may  be  started  by,  but 
in  their  carrying  out  are  guided  by  and  coordinated  by  afferent 
impulses  along  afferent  nerves,  including  those  of  the  special  senses. 
But  in  the  case  of  the  rabbit  it  is  even  still  clearer  than  in  the  case 
of  the  bird  that  the  effects  of  these  afferent  impulses  are  different 
from  those  which  result  when  the  impulses  gain  access  to  an 
intact  brain.  The  movements  of  the  animal  seem  guided  by 
impressions  made  on  its  retina,  as  well  as  on  other  sensory  nerves ; 
we  may  perhaps  speak  of  the  animal  as  the  subject  of  sensations ; 
but  there  is  no  satisfactory  evidence  that  it  possesses  either  visual 
or  other  perceptions,  or  that  the  sensations  which  it  experiences  give 
rise  to  ideas.  Its  avoidance  of  objects  depends  not  so  much  on 
the  form  of  these  as  on  their  interference  with  light.  No  image, 
whether  pleasant  or  terrible,  whether  of  food  or  of  an  enemy, 
produces  an  effect  on  it,  other  than  that  of  an  object  reflecting 
more  or  less  light.  And  we  may  infer  that  it  lacks  the  possession 
of  an  intelligent  will.  But  it  must  always  be  remembered  that 
some  of  the  phenomena  are  due  to  the  operation  producing  other 
results  than  the  mere  absence  of  the  part  removed.  We  must 
bear  in  mind  that  in  all  the  above  experiments  while  the  positive 
phenomena,  the  things  which  the  animal  continues  able  to  do, 
are  of  great  value,  the  negative  phenomena,  the  things  which  the 
animal  can  no  longer  do,  are  of  much  less,  indeed  of  doubtful 
value.  The  more  carefully  and  successfully  the  experiments 
are  carried  out,  the  narrower  become  what  we  may  call  the 
'deficiency  phenomena,'  the  phenomena  which  are  alone  and 
directly  due  to  something  having  been  taken  away.  Were  it 
possible  to  keep  the  rabbit  alive  long  enough  for  the  mere  effects 
of  the  operation  to  pass  completely  away,  we  should  not  only 
probably  witness,  as  in  the  case  of  the  bird,  a  greater  scope  of 
movement  and  more  frequent  spontaneity,  but  possibly  find  a 
difficulty  in  describing  the  exact  condition  of  the  animal. 

§  641.  Hitherto  attempts  to  witness  similar  phenomena  in 
more  highly  organised  mammals  such  as  the  dog  have  failed ;  these 
animals  do  not  recover  from  the  operation  of  removing  the  whole 


CHAP,  ii.]  THE   BRAIN.  1007 

of  both  their  hemispheres  sufficiently  to  enable  us  to  judge 
whether  they,  like  the  frog,  the  bird  and  the  rabbit,  can  carry  out 
coordinate  bodily  movements  in  the  absence  of  the  hemispheres,  or 
whether  in  them  this  part  of  the  brain,  so  largely  developed,  has 
usurped  functions  which  in  the  lower  animals  belong  to  other 
parts.  Our  knowledge  is  largely  confined  to  the  experience  that 
when  in  a  dog  the  cerebral  convolutions  are  removed  piecemeal 
at  several  operations,  the  animal  may  be  kept  alive  and  in  good 
health  for  a  long  time,  many  months  at  least,  even  after  these 
parts  of  the  brain  have  been  reduced  to  very  small  dimensions, 
and  that  under  these  circumstances,  the  animal  is  not  only  able 
to  carry  out  with  some  limitations  his  ordinary  bodily  movements, 
but  also  exhibits  a  spontaneity  obviously  betokening  the  possession 
not  merely  of  a  conscious  volition  but  of  a  certain  amount  of 
intelligence.  Unless  we  are  willing  to  believe  that  a  mere 
fragment  so  to  speak  of  the  hemispheres  can  take  on  most 
extended  powers,  such  an  experience  seems  to  shew  that  in  the 
dog  as  in  the  rabbit  and  in  the  bird,  the  development  of  so-called 
higher  functions  is  not  limited  to  the  cerebral  hemispheres,  that 
the  middle  and  lower  portions  of  the  brain  in  the  higher  animals 
as  compared  with  the  lower  do  not  increase  in  bulk  merely  as  the 
instruments  of  the  hemispheres,  but  like  the  hemispheres  acquire 
more  and  more  complex  functions.  We  may  perhaps  go  so 
far  as  to  ask  the  question  whether  the  volition  and  intelligence 
which  such  a  dog  exhibits  is  not  as  much  the  product  of  the 
parts  lying  behind  the  hemispheres  as  of  the  stump  left  in  the 
front. 

If  we  can  thus  say  little  about  the  condition  of  a  dog  without 
the  cerebral  hemispheres  we  can  say  still  less  about  the  monkey, 
which  in  all  matters  touching  the  cerebral  nervous  system  serves 
as  our  best,  indeed  our  only  guide  for  drawing  inferences  concern- 
ing man ;  but  in  all  probability  the  monkey  in  this  respect  bears 
somewhat  the  same  relation  to  the  dog  that  the  dog  bears  to  the 
bird. 

In  short,  the  more  we  study  the  phenomena  exhibited  by 
animals  possessing  a  part  only  of  their  brain,  the  closer  we  are 
pushed  to  the  conclusion  that  no  sharp  line  can  be  drawn  between 
volition  and  the  lack  of  volition,  or  between  the  possession  and 
absence  of  intelligence.  Between  the  muscle-nerve  preparation 
at  the  one  limit,  and  our  conscious  willing  selves  at  the  other, 
there  is  a  continuous  gradation  without  a  break ;  we  cannot  fix  on 
any  linear  barrier  in  the  brain  or  in  the  general  nervous  system, 
and  say  *  beyond  this  there  is  volition  and  intelligence  but  up  to 
this  there  is  none/ 

This   however  is  not  the  question  with  which   we  are  now 

dealing.     What   we   want   to   point   out   is   that   in  the  higher 

animals,  including  at  least  some  mammals,  as  in  the  frog,  after 

the  removal  of  the  cerebral  hemispheres,  even  though  conscious 

F.  64 


1008         WITHOUT   CEREBRAL   HEMISPHERES.       [Boon  in. 

volition  and  intelligence  appear  to  be  largely,  if  not  entirely,  lost, 
the  body  is  still  capable  of  executing  all  the  ordinary  movements 
which  the  animal  in  its  natural  life  is  wont  to  perform,  in  spite  of 
these  movements  necessitating  the  cooperation  of  various  afferent 
impulses ;  and  that  therefore  the  nervous  machinery  for  the 
execution  of  these  movements  lies  in  some  part  of  the  brain 
other  than  the  cerebral  hemispheres.  We  have  reasons  for 
thinking  that  it  is  situated  in  the  structures  forming  the  middle 
and  hind  brain ;  as  we  shall  see,  interference  with  these  parts 
produces  at  once  remarkable  disorders  of  movement. 


SEC.    5.     THE   MACHINERY   OF   COORDINATED 
MOVEMENTS. 


§  642.  We  may  now  direct  our  attention  for  a  while  to  some 
considerations  concerning  the  nature  of  this  complex  nervous 
machinery  for  the  coordination  of  bodily  movements,  and  espe- 
cially concerning  the  part  played  by  afferent  impulses.  Most  of 
our  knowledge  on  this  point  has  been  gained  by  a  study  of  animals 
not  deprived  of,  but  still  possessing  their  cerebral  hemispheres,  or 
by  deductions  from  the  data  of  our  own  experience;  but  it  is 
possible  in  most  cases  to  eliminate  from  the  total  results  the 
phenomena  which  are  due  to  the  working  of  a  conscious  intelli- 

fence.  Some  of  the  most  striking  facts  bearing  on  this  matter 
ave  been  gained  by  studying  the  effects  of  operative  interference 
with  certain  parts  of  the  internal  ear,  known  as  the  semicircular 
canals.  The  details  of  the  structure  of  these  parts  we  shall 
describe  later  on  when  we  come  to  deal  with  hearing,  but  we 
may  here  say  that  each  internal  ear  possesses  three  membranous 
semicircular  canals,  disposed  in  the  three  planes  of  space  (one 
horizontal,  and  one  in  each  of  the  two  vertical  planes,  fore  and 
aft  and  side  to  side),  each  membranous  canal  being  surrounded 
by  a  bony  canal  of  nearly  the  same  shape,  and  being  expanded 
at  one  end  into  what  is  called  an  ampulla,  on  which  fibres  of 
the  auditory  nerve  end.  Each  membranous  canal,  in  common 
with  the  cavity  of  the  internal  ear  of  which  it  is  a  prolongation, 
contains  a  fluid  allied  to  lymph,  called  endolymph,  and  the  space 
between  each  membranous  canal  and  its  corresponding  bony  canal 
is  in  reality  a  lymph  space,  containing  a  fluid  which  is  virtually 
lymph,  though  it  is  called  by  the  special  name  of  perilymph. 
In  birds  interference  with  the  semicircular  canals  produces  the 
following  remarkable  results. 

When  in  a  pigeon  the  horizontal  membranous  semicircular 
canal  is  cut  through,  the  bird  is  observed  to  be  continually  moving 
its  head  from  side  to  side.  If  one  of  the  vertical  canals  be  cut 
through,  the  movements  are  up  and  down.  The  peculiar  move- 
ments may  not  be  witnessed  when  the  bird  is  perfectly  quiet,  but 

64—2 


1010  SEMICIRCULAR   CANALS.  [BOOK  in. 

they  make  their  appearance  whenever  it  is  disturbed,  or  attempts 
in  any  way  to  stir.  When  the  injury  is  confined  to  one  canal  only 
or  even  to  the  canals  of  one  side  of  the  head  only,  the  condition 
after  a  while  passes  away ;  when  the  canals  of  both  sides  have 
been  divided,  it  becomes  much  exaggerated,  lasts  much  longer,  and 
in  some  cases  is  said  to  remain  permanently.  After  such  injuries 
it  is  found  that  these  peculiar  movements  of  the  head  are  asso- 
ciated with  what  appears  to  be  a  great  want  of  coordination  of 
bodily  movements.  If  the  bird  be  thrown  into  the  air,  it  flutters 
and  falls  down  in  a  helpless  and  confused  manner ;  it  appears  to 
have  lost  the  power  of  orderly  flight.  If  placed  in  a  balanced 
position,  it  may  remain  for  some  time  quiet,  generally  with  its 
head  in  a  peculiar  posture ;  but  directly  it  is  disturbed,  the 
movements  which  it  attempts  to  execute  are  irregular  and  fall 
short  of  their  purpose.  It  has  great  difficulty  in  picking  up  food 
and  in  drinking;  and  in  general  its  behaviour  very  much  re- 
sembles that  of  a  person  who  is  exceedingly  dizzy. 

It  can  hear  perfectly  well,  and  therefore  the  symptoms  cannot 
be  regarded  as  the  result  of  any  abnormal  auditory  sensations,  such 
as  '  a  roaring'  in  the  ears.  Besides,  any  such  stimulation  of  the 
auditory  nerve  as  the  result  of  the  section  would  speedily  die  away, 
whereas  these  phenomena  may  last  for  at  least  a  very  considerable 
time. 

The  movements  are  not  occasioned  by  any  partial  paralysis,  by 
any  want  of  power  in  particular  muscles  or  group  of  muscles; 
though  removal  of  the  canals  of  one  side  has  been  described  as 
leading  to  diminished  muscular  force  on  the  same  side  of  the 
body,  the  mere  diminution  of  force  is  insufficient  to  explain  the 
phenomena.  Nor  on  the  other  hand  are  the  movements  due  to 
any  uncontrollable  impulse ;  a  very  gentle  pressure  of  the  hand 
suffices  to  stop  the  movements  of  the  head,  and  the  hand  in  doing 
so  experiences  no  strain.  The  assistance  of  a  very  slight  support 
enables  movements  otherwise  impossible  or  most  difficult,  to  be 
easily  executed.  Thus,  though  when  left  alone  the  bird  has  great 
difficulty  in  drinking  or  picking  up  corn,  it  will  continue  to  drink 
or  eat  with  ease  if  its  beak  be  plunged  into  water,  or  into  a  heap 
of  barley ;  the  slight  support  of  the  water  or  of  the  grain  seems 
sufficient  to  steady  its  movements.  In  the  same  way  it  can, 
even  without  assistance,  clean  its  feathers  and  scratch  its  head, 
its  beak  and  foot  being  in  these  operations  guided  by  contact 
with  its  own  body. 

The  amount  of  disorder  thus  induced  differs  in  different  birds ; 
and  some  movements  are  more  affected  than  others.  As  a  general 
rule  it  may  be  said  that  the  more  complex  and  intricate  a  move- 
ment, the  fuller  and  more  delicate  the  coordination  needed  to 
carry  it  out  successfully,  the  more  markedly  is  it  disordered  by 
the  operation;  thus  after  injury  to  the  canals,  while  a  pigeon 
cannot  fly,  a  goose  is  still  able  to  swim. 


CHAP,  ii.]  THE   BRAIN.  1011 

In  mammals  (rabbits)  section  of  the  canals  also  produces  a 
certain  amount  of  loss  of  coordination,  but  much  less  than  that 
witnessed  in  birds ;  and  the  movements  of  the  head  are  not  so 
marked,  peculiar  oscillating  movements  of  the  eyeballs,  differing  in 
direction  and  character  according  to  the  canal  or  canals  operated 
upon,  becoming  however  prominent.  In  the  frog  no  deviations  of 
the  head  are  seen,  but  there  is  some  loss  of  coordination  in  the 
movements  of  the  body.  In  fishes  no  effect  at  all  is  produced. 

Injury  to  the  bony  canals  alone  is  insufficient  to  produce  the 
symptoms;  the  membranous  canals  themselves  must  be  divided 
or  injured.  The  characteristic  movements  of  the  head  may 
however  be  brought  about  in  a  bird  without  opening  the  bony 
canal,  by  suddenly  heating  or  cooling  a  canal,  especially  its 
ampullar  terminations,  or  by  the  making  or  breaking  of  a  con- 
stant current  directed  through  the  canal. 

There  can  be  no  doubt  that  these  characteristic  movements 
of  the  head  are  the  result  of  afferent  impulses  started  in  the 
nervous  endings  of  the  auditory  nerve  over  the  ampulla  of  the 
canal,  and  conveyed  to  the  brain  along  that  nerve.  And  that 
injury  to  or  other  stimulation  of  each  of  the  three  canals  should 
produce  in  each  case  a  different  movement  of  the  head,  the 
direction  of  the  movement  being  different  according  to  the  plane 
in  which  the  canal  lies,  shews  that  these  impulses  are  of  a  peculiar 
nature.  This  is  further  illustrated  by  the  following  experiment. 
If  the  horizontal  canal  be  carefully  laid  bare,  and  the  membranous 
canal  opened  so  as  to  expose  the  endolymph,  blowing  gently  over 
the  opened  canal  with  a  fine  glass  cannula  will  produce  a  definite 
movement  of  the  head,  which  is  turned  to  the  one  side  or  to  the 
other,  according  as  the  current  of  air  drives  the  endolymph 
towards  or  away  from  the  ampulla.  From  this  it  is  inferred 
that  a  movement  of  the  endolymph  over,  or  an  increased  pressure 
of  the  endolymph  on,  the  nervous  endings  in  the  ampulla  gives 
rise  to  afferent  impulses  which  in  some  way  determine  the  issue 
of  efferent  impulses  leading  to  the  movement  of  the  head.  It  is 
further  suggested  that  since  the  planes  of  the  three  canals  lie  in 
the  three  axes  of  space,  any  change  in  the  position  of  the  head 
must  lead  to  changes  in  the  pressure  of  the  endolymph  on  the 
walls  of  the  ampullae  or  to  movements  of  endolymph  over  those 
walls,  and  so  must  give  rise  to  impulses  passing  up  the  auditory 
nerve ;  and  that  since  every  change  of  position  will  affect  the  three 
canals  differently  (whereas  the  changes  of  pressure  of  the  endo- 
lymph involved  in  a  "  wave  of  sound"  will  affect  all  three  ampullae 
equally)  those  impulses  will  differ  according  to  the  direction  of 
the  change.  A  still  further  extension  of  this  view  supposes  that 
since  in  any  one  position  of  the  head  the  pressure  of  the  endo- 
lymph will  differ  in  the  three  ampullae,  mere  position  of  the  head, 
as  distinguished  from  change  of  position,  is  adequate  to  generate 
afferent  impulses  differing  in  the  different  positions. 


1012  SEMICIRCULAR   CANALS.  [BOOK  m. 

Let  us  now  for  a  while  turn  aside  to  ourselves  and  examine 
the  coordination  of  the  movements  of  our  own  bodies.  When  we 
appeal  to  our  own  consciousness  we  find  that  our  movements  are 
governed  and  guided  by  what  we  may  call  a  sense  of  equilibrium, 
by  an  appreciation  of  the  position  of  our  body  and  its  relations  to 
space.  When  this  sense  of  equilibrium  is  disturbed  we  say  we 
are  dizzy,  and  we  then  stagger  and  reel,  being  no  longer  able  to 
coordinate  the  movements  of  our  bodies  or  to  adapt  them  to  the 
position  of  things  around  us.  What  is  the  origin  of  this  sense 
of  equilibrium  ?  By  what  means  are  we  able  to  appreciate  the 
position  of  our  body?  There  can  be  no  doubt  that  this  appre- 
ciation is  in  large  measure  the  product  of  visual  and  tactile 
sensations ;  we  recognize  the  relations  of  our  body  to  the  things 
around  us  in  great  measure  by  sight  and  touch ;  we  also  learn 
much  by  our  muscular  sense.  But  there  is  something  besides 
these.  Neither  sight  nor  touch  nor  muscular  sense  can  help  us 
when,  placed  perfectly  flat  and  at  rest  on  a  horizontal  rotating 
table,  with  the  eyes  shut  and  not  a  muscle  stirring,  we  attempt  to 
determine  whether  or  no  the  table  and  we  with  it  are  being  moved, 
or  to  ascertain  how  much  it  and  we  are  turned  to  the  right  or  to 
the  left.  Yet  under  such  circumstances  we  are  conscious  of  a 
change  in  our  position,  and  some  observers  have  been  even  able  to 
pass  a  tolerably  successful  judgment  as  to  the  angle  through  which 
they  have  been  moved.  There  can  be  no  doubt  that  such  a 
judgment  is  based  upon  the  interpretation  by  consciousness  of 
afferent  impulses  which  are  dependent  on  the  position  of  the 
body,  but  which  are  not  afferent  impulses  belonging  to  sensations 
of  touch  or  sight,  or  taking  part  in  the  muscular  sense.  And  it 
is  urged  with  great  plausibility  that  the  afferent  impulses  in 
question  are  those  which  we  have  just  referred  to  as  started  in 
the  semicircular  canals. 

If  we  admit  the  existence  of  such  ampullar  impulses,  if  we 
may  venture  so  to  call  them,  and  recognise  them  as  contributing 
largely  not  only  to  our  direct  perception  of  the  position  of  the 
head  and  thus  of  the  body,  but  also  in  a  more  indirect  way  to 
what  we  have  called  the  sense  of  equilibrium,  we  should  expect  to 
find  that  when  they  are  abnormal  the  sense  of  equilibrium  is 
disturbed,  and  that  in  consequence  a  failure  of  coordination  in  our 
movements  results.  And  the  loss  of  coordination  which  we 
described  above  as  resulting  from  injury  to  the  semicircular 
canals  has  accordingly  been  attributed  to  a  deficiency  or  disorder 
of  normal  ampullar  impulses. 

But  we  must  here  distinguish  between  two  things.  It  seems 
clear  that  when  the  membranous  canals  are  injured  or  otherwise 
stimulated  afferent  impulses  are  generated  which  on  the  one  hand 
may  produce  peculiar  movements  of  the  head,  and  on  the  other 
hand  seem  able  when  the  injury  is  large  to  cause  a  loss  of  coordi- 
nation of  bodily  movements.  But  it  does  not  necessarily  follow 


CHAP,  ii.]  THE   BRAIN.  1013 

from  this  that  in  a  normal  condition  of  things  afferent  impulses 
are  continually  passing  up  to  the  brain  from  the  semicircular 
canals,  and  that  the  loss  of  coordination  which  follows  upon  injury 
to  the  canals  is  due  to  these  normal  impulses  being  deficient  or 
altered.  It  may  be  that  such  normal  impulses  do  not  exist,  and 
that  the  loss  of  coordination  is  the  result  of  the  central  machinery 
for  coordination  being  interfered  with  by  quite  new  impulses  gene- 
rated by  the  injury  to  the  canal  with  the  consequent  loss  of  endo- 
lymph  acting  as  a  stimulus  to  the  endings  of  the  nerve.  For  the 
experience  quoted  above,  though  it  proves  that  afferent  impulses 
other  than  those  of  sight,  touch  and  the  muscular  sense  do  reach 
the  brain  and  afford  a  basis  for  a  judgment  as  to  the  position 
of  the  body,  does  not  by  itself  prove  that  those  impulses  come 
from  the  semicircular  canals ;  the  arrangement  of  the  canals  is 
undoubtedly  suggestive ;  but  it  is  quite  possible  that  the  afferent 
impulses  in  question  may  be  generated  by  one  or  other  of  various 
changes,  vaso-motor  and  others,  of  the  tissues  of  the  body  which 
are  involved  in  a  change  of  position.  And  if  it  be  true  as  affirmed 
by  some  observers  that  both  auditory  nerves  may  be  completely 
and  permanently  severed,  without  any  effect  on  the  coordination 
of  movements,  it  is  obvious  that  the  incoordination  which  follows 
upon  section  of  the  semicircular  canals  is  due  to  some  special 
irritation  set  up  by  the  operation  and  not  to  the  mere  absence  of 
any  normal  ampullar  impulses.  On  the  other  hand,  if  the  effects 
are  those  of  irritation,  it  is  difficult  to  understand  how  they  can, 
as  according  to  certain  observers  they  certainly  do,  become  per- 
manent. It  has  however  been  strongly  urged  that  in  such  cases 
of  permanent  incoordination,  the  operation  has  set  up  secondary 
mischief  in  the  brain,  in  the  cerebellum  for  instance,  with  which 
as  we  have  seen  (§  618)  the  vestibular  auditory  nerve  makes 
special  connections,  and  that  the  permanent  effects  are  really  due 
to  the  disease  going  on  here;  and  we  have  reason,  as  we  shall 
see,  to  think  that  the  cerebellum  is  concerned  in  the  coordination 
of  movements.  It  cannot  therefore  be  regarded  as  settled  that 
the  canals  are  the  source  of  normal  impulses,  or  that  our  conscious 
appreciation  of  the  position  of  the  head  and  so  of  the  body  in 
space  is  based  on  such  impulses.  But  such  a  view  is  not  dis- 
proved ;  and  in  any  case  it  remains  true  that  injury  to  the  canals 
does  in  some  way  or  other,  either  by  generating  new  impulses 
or  by  altering  preexisting  ones,  so  modify  the  flow  of  'afferent 
impulses  into  the  machinery  of  coordination  as  to  throw  that 
machinery  out  of  gear. 

§  643.  We  have  dwelt  on  these  phenomena  of  the  semicircular 
canals  because  they  illustrate  in  a  striking  manner  the  important 
part  played  by  afferent  impulses  in  the  coordination  of  movements. 
We  saw  reason  to  think  (§  589)  that  even  in  an  ordinary  reflex 
movement  carried  out  by  the  spinal  cord  or  by  a  portion  of  the 
cord  afferent  impulses,  other  than  those  which  excite  the  movement, 


101 4  MACHINERY   OF  COORDINATION.         [BOOK  HI. 

are  at  work,  determining  such  coordination  as  is  present.  In  such 
a  case  the  coordinating  afferent  impulses  are  relatively  simple 
in  character  and  start  chiefly  at  all  events  in  the  muscles  con- 
cerned. In  an  animal  possessing  the  lower  parts  of  the  brain, 
though  deprived  of  the  cerebral  hemispheres,  the  coordinating 
afferent  impulses,  in  accordance  with  the  greater  diversity  and 
complexity  of  the  movements  which  the  animal  is  able  to  execute, 
are  far  more  potent  and  varied.  Besides  afferent  impulses  from 
the  muscles,  forming  the  basis  of  what  we  have  called  the  muscular 
sense,  afferent  impulses  from  the  skin,  forming  the  basis  of  the 
sense  of  touch  in  the  wide  meaning  of  that  word,  other  afferent 
impulses  of  obscure  character  from  the  viscera  and  various  tissues, 
and  the  peculiar  afferent  ampullar  impulses  of  which  we  have  just 
spoken,  important  special  afferent  impulses  borne  along  the  nerves 
of  sight  and  hearing  come  into  play.  The  frog,  the  bird,  and  even 
the  mammal,  deprived  of  the  cerebral  hemispheres,  though  it  may 
shew  little  signs  or  none  at  all  of  having  a  distinct  volition,  is  as 
we  have  urged  indubitably  affected  by  visual  and  auditory 
impressions,  and  whether  we  admit  or  no  that  such  an  animal 
can  rightly  be  spoken  of  as  being  conscious  we  cannot  resist  the 
conclusion  that  afferent  impulses  started  in  its  retina  or  internal 
ear  produce  in  its  central  nervous  system  changes  similar  to  those 
which  in  a  conscious  animal  form  the  basis  of  visual  and  auditory 
sensations,  and  we  must  either  call  these  changes  sensations  or 
find  for  them  some  new  word.  Whatever  we  call  them,  and 
whether  consciousness  is  distinctly  involved  in  them  or  no,  they 
obviously  play  an  important  part  as  factors  of  the  coordination 
of  movements.  Indeed,  when  we  appeal  to  the  experience  of 
ourselves  in  possession  of  consciousness,  we  find ,_  that  though 
various  sensations  clearly  enter  into  the  coordination  of  our 
movements,  we  carry  out  movements  thus  coordinated  without 
being  distinctly  aware  of  these  coordinating  factors.  In  every 
movement  which  we  make  the  coordination  of  the  movement  is 
dependent  on  the  impulses  or  influences  which  form  the  basis 
of  the  muscular  sense,  yet  we  are  not  distinctly  conscious  of 
these  impulses;  it  is  only  as  we  shall  see  by  special  analysis 
that  we  come  to  the  conclusion  that  we  do  possess  what  we 
shall  call  a  muscular  sense.  So  again,  taking  the  matter  from 
a  somewhat  different  point  of  view,  many  of  our  movements, 
markedly  as  we  shall  see  those  of  the  eyeballs,  are  coordinated 
by  visual  sensations,  and  when  we  sing  or  when  we  dance  to 
music  our  movements  are  coordinated  by  the  help  of  sensations 
of  sound.  In  these  cases  distinct  sensations  in  the  ordinary  sense 
of  the  word  intervene ;  if  we  cannot  see  or  cannot  hear,  the 
movement  fails  or  is  imperfect ;  yet  even  in  these  cases  we  are 
not  directly  conscious  of  the  sensations  as  coordinating  factors ;  it 
needs  careful  analysis  to  prove  that  the  success  of  the  movement 
is'  really  dependent  on  the  sound  or  on  the  sight.  These  and 


CHAP.  IL]  THE   BRAIN.  1015 

other  facts  suggest  the  view  that  the  point  at  whiclp.  the  various 
afferent  impulses  which  form  the  basis  of  the  sensations  of  a^ 
conscious  individual  enter  into  the  coordinating  mechanism  is  or 
may  be  some  way  short  of  the  stage  at  which  the  cotn^ete 
conversion  of  the  impulse  into  a  perfect  sensation  takes  place. 
The  events  which  constitute  what  we  may  call  visual  impulses,  as 
these  leave  the  retina  to  sweep  along  the  optic  nerve,  are  we  must 
admit  very  different  from  those  which  in  the  appropriate  parts  of  the 
brain  constitute  what  we  may  call  conscious  vision ;  and  probably 
between  the  beginning  and  the  end  there  are  progressive  changes. 
It  is  probable,  we  say,  that  these  visual  events  may  affect  the 
coordinating  mechanism  at  some  stage  of  their  progress  before 
they  reach  their  final  and  perfect  form.  If  this  be  so  we  may 
further  conclude  that  though,  when  the  whole  nervous  machinery 
is  present  in  its  entirety,  the  afferent  impulses  which  take  part  in 
coordination  must  inevitably  at  the  same  time  give  rise  to 
conscious  sensations,  they  might  still  effect  their  coordinating 
work  when,  owing  to  the  imperfection  or  lack  of  the  terminal  part 
of  the  nervous  machinery,  the  impulses  failed  to  receive  their  final 
transformation,  and  conscious  sensations  were  absent.  In  other 
words  the  coordinating  influences  of  sensory  or  afferent  impulses 
are  not  essentially  dependent  on  the  existence  of  a  distinct 
consciousness. 

§  644.  We  have  raised  this  point  partly  for  the  sake  of  illus- 
trating the  working  of  the  coordination  machinery  in  the  absence 
of  the  cerebral  hemispheres,  but  also  in  order  to  aid  in  the  inter- 
pretation of  the  subjective  condition  which  we  speak  of  as 
giddiness  or  dizziness  or  vertigo.  We  compared  the  condition  of 
the  pigeon  after  an  injury  to  the  semicircular  canals  to  that  of  a 
person  who  is  giddy  or  dizzy,  and  indeed  vertigo  is  the  subjective 
expression  of  a  disarrangement  of  the  coordination  machinery, 
especially  of  that  concerned  in  the  maintenance  of  bodily  equili- 
brium. It  may  be  brought  about  in  many  ways.  When  a  constant 
current  of  adequate  strength  is  sent  through  the  head  from  ear  to 
ear,  we  experience  a  sense  of  vertigo ;  our  movements  then  appear 
to  a  bystander  to  fail  in  coordination,  in  fact  to  resemble  those  of 
a  pigeon  whose  semicircular  canals  have  been  injured ;  and  indeed 
the  effects  are  probably  produced  in  the  same  way  in  the  two- 
cases.  In  what  is  called  Meniere's  disease  attacks  of  vertigo  seem 
to  be  associated  with  disease  in  the  ear,  being  attributed  by  many 
to  disorder  of  the  semicircular  canals,  and  cases  have  been  re- 
corded of  giddiness  as  well  as  deafness  resulting  from  disease 
of  the  auditory  nerve.  Visual  sensations  are  very  potent  in 
producing  vertigo.  Many  persons  feel  giddy  when  they  look  at  a 
waterfall ;  and  this  is  a  case  in  which  both  the  sense  of  giddiness 
and  the  disarrangement  of  coordination  is  the  result  of  the  action 
of  a  pure  sensation  and  nothing  else.  In  the  well-known  intense 
vertigo  which  is  caused  by  rapid  rotation  of  the  body  visual 


1016  MACHINERY   OF   COORDINATION.         [BOOK  in. 

sensation  plays  a  part  when  the  rotation  is  carried  on  with  the  eyes 
open,  but  only  a  part ;  for  vertigo  may  be  induced,  though  not  so 
readily,  by  rotation  with  the  eyes  completely  shut.  In  the  latter 
case  it  has  been  suggested  that  the  vertigo  is  caused  by  abnormal 
ampullar  impulses,  but  these  can  only  contribute  to  the  result 
which  is  in  the  main  caused  by  direct  disturbance  of  the  brain. 
When  the  rotation  is  carried  out  with  the  eyes  open,  the  vertigo 
which  is  felt  when  the  rotation  ceases  is  partly  caused  by  the 
visual  sensations,  on  account  of  the  behaviour  of  the  eyeballs, 
ceasing  to  be  in  harmony  with  the  rest  of  the  sensations  and 
afferent  impulses  which  help  to  make  up  the  coordination.  The 
rotation  sets  up  peculiar  oscillating  movements  of  the  eyeballs, 
which  continue  for  some  time  after  the  rotation  has  ceased ;  owing 
to  these  movements  of  the  eyeballs  the  visual  sensations  excited 
are  such  as  would  be  excited  if  external  objects  were  rapidly 
moving,  whereas  all  the  other  sensations  and  impulses  which  are 
affecting  the  central  nervous  system  are  such  as  are  excited  by 
objects  at  rest.  In  a  normal  state  of  things  the  visual  and  the 
other  sensations  and  impulses,  which  go  to  make  up  the  coordina- 
ting machinery,  are  in  accord  with  each  other  in  reference  to  the 
events  in  the  external  world  which  are  giving  rise  to  them ;  after 
rotation  they  are  for  a  time  in  disaccord,  and  the  coordinating 
machinery  is  in  consequence  disarranged. 

When  we  interrogate  our  own  consciousness,  we  find  that  we 
are  not  distinctly  conscious  of  this  disaccord ;  the  visual  sensations 
are  so  prepotent  in  consciousness,  that  we  really  think  the  external 
world  is  rapidly  whirling  round ;  all  that  we  are  further  conscious 
of  is  the  feeling  of  giddiness  and  our  inability  to  make  our  bodily 
movements  harmonize  with  our  visual  sensations.  So  that  even  in 
the  cases  where  the  loss  of  coordination  is  brought  about  by 
distinct  sensations  what  we  really  appreciate  by  means  of  our 
consciousness  is  the  disarrangement  of  the  coordinating  machinery. 
It  is  the  appreciation  of  this  disorder  which  constitutes  the  feeling 
of  vertigo ;  both  the  feeling  of  giddiness  and  the  disordered  move- 
ments are  the  outcome,  one  subjective  and  the  other  objective,  of 
the  same  thing.  It  is  not  because  we  feel  giddy  that  we  stagger 
and  reel ;  our  movements  are  wrong  because  the  machinery  is  at 
fault,  and  it  is  the  faulty  action  of  the  machinery  which  also  makes 
us  feel  giddy. 

We  may  here  perhaps  remark  that  it  is  an  actually  disordered 
condition  of  the  coordinating  mechanism  which  gives  rise  to  the 
affection  of  consciousness  which  we  call  giddiness,  not  a  mere  cur- 
tailing of  the  mechanism  or  any  failure  on  its  part  to  make 
itself  effective.  Complete  blindness  limits  the  range  of  activity 
of  the  machinery  but  leaves  the  remainder  intact,  and  no  giddi- 
ness is  felt.  So  again  in  certain  diseases  of  the  nervous  system 
the  muscular  sense  is  interfered  with  over  considerable  regions 
of  the  body,  and  in  these  regions  coordination  fails  or  is  imperfect, 


CHAP.  IL]  THE   BRAIN.  1017 

but  the  central  machinery  is  not  thereby  affected,  though  its  area 
of  usefulness  is  limited,  and  no  giddiness  is  experienced ;  and  so 
in  other  instances. 

§  645.  Forced  Movements.  So  far  we  have  dwelt  on  disorders 
of  the  coordinating  machinery  brought  about  by  the  action  of 
various  afferent  impulses.  We  have  now  to  call  attention  to  some 
peculiar  phenomena  which  result  from  operative  interference  with 
parts  of  the  brain,  and  which  in  some  instances  at  least  may  be 
taken  to  illustrate  how  this  complex  machinery  works  when  some 
of  its  inner  wheels  are  broken. 

All  investigators  who  have  performed  experiments  on  the 
brain  have  observed,  as  the  result  of  injury  to  various  parts 
of  it,  remarkable  movements  which  have  the  appearance  of  being 
irresistible,  compulsory,  forced.  They  vary  much  in  the  extent 
to  which  they  are  developed ;  some  are  so  slight  as  hardly  to  deserve 
the  name,  while  others  are  strikingly  intense.  One  of  the  most 
common  forms  is  that  in  which  the  animal  rolls  incessantly  round 
the  longitudinal  axis  of  its  own  body.  This  is  especially  common 
after  section  of  one  of  the  crura  cerebri,  or  of  the  middle  and 
inferior  peduncles  of  the  cerebellum,  or  after  unilateral  section 
of  the  pons,  but  has  also  been  witnessed  after  injury  to  the  bulb 
and  corpora  quadrigemina.  Sometimes  the  animal  rotates  towards 
and  sometimes  away  from  the  side  operated  on.  Another  form  is 
that  in  which  the  animal  executes  '  circus  movements/  i.e.  con- 
tinually moves  round  and  round  in  a  circle  of  longer  or  shorter 
radius,  sometimes  towards  and  sometimes  away  from  the  injured 
side.  This  may  be  seen  after  several  of  the  above-mentioned 
operations,  and  in  one  form  or  another  is  not  uncommon  after 
various  unilateral  injuries  to  the  brain.  There  is  a  variety  of  the 
circus  movement,  "the  clockhand  movement,"  said  to  occur 
frequently  after  lesions  of  the  posterior  corpora  quadrigemina,  in 
which  the  animal  moves  in  a  circle,  with  the  longitudinal  axis  of 
its  body  as  a  radius,  and  the  end  of  its  tail  for  a  centre.  And  this 
form  again  may  easily  pass  into  a  simple  rolling  movement.  In 
yet  another  form  the  animal  rotates  over  the  transverse  axis  of  its 
body,  tumbles  head  over  heels  in  a  series  of  somersaults  ;  or  it  may 
run  incessantly  in  a  straight  line  backwards  or  forwards  until  it  is 
stopped  by  some  obstacle.  These  latter  forms  of  forced  movements 
are  sometimes  seen  after  injury  to  the  corpus  striatum  even  when 
a  very  limited  portion  of  the  grey  matter  is  affected.  And  many 
of  these  forced  movements  may  result  from  injuries  which  appear 
to  be  confined  to  the  cerebral  cortex. 

When  the  phenomena  are  well  developed,  every  effort  of  the 
animal  brings  on  a  movement  of  this  forced  character.  Left  to 
itself  and  at  rest  the  animal  may  present  nothing  abnormal,  its 
posture  and  attitude  may  be  quite  natural ;  but  when  it  is  excited 
to  move  or  when  it  attempts  of  itself  to  move,  it  executes  not 
a  natural  movement  but  a  forced  one,  turning  round  or  rolling 


1018  FORCED   MOVEMENTS.  [BOOK  m. 

over  as  the  case  may  be.  In  severe  cases  the  movement  is 
continued  until  the  animal  is  exhausted ;  when  the  exhaustion 
passes  off  the  animal  may  remain  for  some  little  time  quiet,  but 
some  stimulus,  intrinsic  or  extrinsic,  soon  inaugurates  a  fresh 
outbreak,  to  be  again  followed  by  exhaustion. 

In  some  of  the  milder  forms,  that  for  instance  of  the  circus 
movement  with  a  long  radius,  the  curved  character  of  the  progres- 
sion appears  simply  due  to  the  fact  that  in  the  effort  of  locomotion 
volitional  impulses  do  not  gain  such  ready  access  to  one  side  of 
the  body  as  to  the  other,  the  injury  having  caused  some  obstacle 
or  other.  Hence  the  contractions  of  the  muscles  of  one  side  (the 
left  for  instance)  of  the  body  are  more  powerful  than  the  other, 
and  in  consequence  the  body  is  continually  thrust  towards  the 
other  (the  right)  side.  As  is  well  known  we  ourselves,  when  our 
walk  is  not  guided  by  visual  sensations,  tend  to  describe  a  circle 
of  somewhat  wide  radius,  the  deviation  being  due  to  a  want  of 
bilateral  symmetry  in  our  limbs ;  and  the  above  circus  movement 
is  only  an  exaggeration  of  this. 

But  the  other  more  intense  forms  of  forced  movements  are 
more  complicated  in  their  nature.  No  mere  blocking  of  volitional 
impulses  will  explain  why  an  animal  whenever  it  attempts  to 
move  rolls  rapidly  over,  or  rushes  irresistibly  forwards  or  back- 
wards. It  is  not  possible  with  our  present  knowledge  to  explain 
how  each  particular  kind  of  movement  is  brought  about;  and 
indeed  the  several  kinds  are  probably  brought  about  in  different 
ways,  for  they  differ  so  greatly  from  each  other  that  we  only  class 
them  together  because  it  is  difficult  to  know  where  to  draw  the 
line  between  them.  But  we  may  regard  the  more  intense  forms 
as  illustrating  the  complex  nature  of  what  we  have  called  the 
coordinating  machinery,  the  capabilities  of  which  are,  so  to 
speak,  disclosed  by  its  being  damaged.  Such  gross  injuries  as  are 
involved  in  dividing  cerebral  structures  or  in  injecting  corrosive 
substances  into  this  or  that  part  of  the  brain,  must,  of  necessity, 
partly  by  blocking  the  way  to  the  impulses  which  in  a  normal 
state  of  things  are  continually  passing  from  one  part  of  the  brain 
to  another,  partly  by  generating  new  unusual  impulses,  seriously 
affect  the  due  working  of  the  general  coordinating  machinery. 
The  fact  that  an  animal  can,  at  any  moment,  by  an  effort  of  its 
own  will,  rotate  on  its  axis  or  run  straight  forwards,  shews  that 
the  nervous  mechanism  for  the  execution  of  those  movements  is 
ready  at  hand  in  the  brain,  waiting  only  to  be  discharged  ;  and  it  is 
easy  to  conceive  how  such  a  discharge  might  be  affected  either  by 
the  substitution  for  the  will  of  some  potent  intrinsic  afferent 
impulse  or  by  some  misdirection  of  volitional  impulses.  Persons 
who  have  experienced  similar  forced  movements  as  the  result  of 
disease  report  that  they  are  frequently  accompanied,  and  seem  to 
be  caused,  by  disturbed  visual  or  other  sensations;  thus  they 
attribute  their  suddenly  falling  forward  to  the  occurrence  of  the 


CHAP.  IL]  THE   BRAIN.  1019 

sensation  that  the  ground  in  front  of  them  is  suddenly  sinking 
away  beneath  their  feet.  Without  trusting  too  closely  to  the 
interpretations  the  subjects  of  these  disorders  give  of  their  own 
feelings,  and  remembering  what  was  said  above  concerning  vertigo, 
we  may  at  least  conclude  that  the  unusual  movements  are  an  many 
cases  due  to  a  disorder  of  the  coordinating  mechanism,  brought 
about  by  strange  or  disordered  sensory  impulses.  And  this  view 
is  supported  by  the  fact  that  many  of  tfrese  forced  movements 
are  accompanied  by  a  peculiar  and  wholly  abnormal  position  of 
the  eyes,  which  alone  might  perhaps  explain  many  of  the  pheno- 
mena. 

§  646.  The  phenomena  presented  by  animals  deprived  of 
their  cerebral  hemispheres  shew  that  this  machinery  of  coordina- 
tion is  supplied  by  cerebral  structures  lying  between  the  cerebral 
hemisphere  above  and  the  top  of  the  spinal  cord  below.  But 
when  we  ask  the  further  question,  how  is  this  machinery  related 
to  the  various  elements  which  go  to  make  up  this  part  of  the 
brain  ?  the  only  answers  which  we  receive  are  of  the  most 
imperfect  kind. 

In  the  case  of  the  frog  we  can,  after  removal  of  the  cerebral 
hemispheres,  make  an  experimental  distinction  in  the  parts  left 
between  the  optic  thalami  with  the  optic  nerves  and  tracts, 
the  optic  lobes,  and  the  bulb  with  the  rudimentary  cerebellum. 
When  the  optic  thalami  are  removed,  as  might  be  expected,  the 
evidence  of  visual  impressions  modifying  the  movements  of  the 
animal  disappears ;  and  it  is  stated  that  apparently  spontaneous 
movements  are  much  more  rare  than  when  the  thalami  are 
intact.  When  the  optic  lobes  as  well  as  the  cerebral  hemispheres 
are  removed,  the  power  of  balancing  is  lost ;  when  such  a  frog  is 
thrown  off  its  balance  by  inclining  the  plane  on  which  it  is  placed, 
it  slips  back  or  falls  down;  the  special  coordinating  mechanism 
for  balancing  must  therefore  in  this  animal  have  a  special 
connection  with  the  optic  lo^tTe^.  But  after  removal  of  these 
organs  the  animal  is  still  capable  of  a  great  variety  of  coordinate 
movements:  unlike  a  frog  retaining  its  spinal  cord  only,  it  can 
swim  and  leap,  it  maintains  a  normal  posture,  and  when  placed  on 
its  back  immediately  regains  the  normal  posture.  The  cerebellum 
of  the  frog  is  so  small,  and  in  removing  it  injury  is  so  likely  to  be 
done  to  the  underlying  parts,  that  it  becomes  difficult  to  say  how 
much  of  the  coordination  apparent  in  a  frog  possessing  cerebellum 
and  bulb  is  to  be  attributed  to  the  former  or  to  the  latter; 
probably,  however,  the  part  played  by  the  former  is  small. 

In  the  case  neither  of  the  bird  nor  of  the  mammal  have  we 
any  exact  information  as  to  the  behaviour  of  the  animal  after 
removal  of  the  parts  behind  the  hemispheres,  in  addition  to  the 
hemispheres  themselves.  Our  knowledge  is  confined  to  the 
results  of  the  ablation,  or  of  the  stimulation  of  parts,  the 
cerebellum  for  instance,  in  animals  in  which  the  rest  of  the  brain 


1020  MACHINERY   OF   COORDINATION.         [BOOK  in. 

has  been  left  intact.  Observations  of  this  kind  have  disclosed 
many  interesting  facts,  besides  the  forced  movements  just  referred 
to,  but  they  have  not  led  to,  and  indeed  could  hardly  be  expected 
to  lead  to,  any  clear  views  as  to  the  point  which  we  are  now  dis- 
cussing. It  does  not  follow  that  every  part,  injury  or  stimulation 
of  which  interferes  with  coordinated  movements,  or  gives  rise  to 
definite,  forced,  or  other  movements,  is  to  be  considered  as  part 
of  the  machinery  under  consideration.  The  corpora  striata  and 
cerebral  hemispheres  form,  as  we  have  seen,  no  part  of  the 
machinery,  yet  injury  to  them  may  disorder  the  machinery ;  and 
the  fact  that  removal  of,  or  injury  to  the  cerebellum,  disorders 
the  machinery  is  no  proof  by  itself  that  the  cerebellum  is  an 
essential  part  of  the  machinery. 

If  we  may  trust  to  deductions  from  structural  arrangements, 
we  might  be  inclined  to  infer  that  the  anatomical  relations  of 
what  we  have  called  the  tegmental  region  from  the  bulb  upwards 
point  to  its  serving  as  the  foundation  of  the  machinery  in 
question.  Behind,  it  has  full  connections  with  various  parts  of 
the  cord,  while  in  front  by  means  of  the  optic  thalami  and 
anterior  corpora  quadrigemina,  if  not  by  other  ways  as  well,  it 
is  so  far  associated  with  the  optic  nerves  that  the  path  seems 
open  for  visual  impulses  to  gain  access  to  it.  To  this  foundation, 
however,  we  must  add  the  cerebellum,  on  account  of  its  relations 
to  it,  to  the  cord  and  to  the  bulb  through  the  restiform  bodies, 
including  its  ties  with  the  auditory  nerve.  And  if  we  add  the 
cerebellum  we  must  also  probably  add  the  pons.  We  may  exclude 
the  pes  of  the  crus,  since  this  is  composed  exclusively  of  fibres 
bringing  the  cerebral  hemispheres,  including  the  corpora  striata, 
into  connection  with  the  pons,  bulb  and  cord,  and  so  with  the 
coordinating  machinery  itself,  .as  well  as  with  other  parts  of  the 
nervous  system.  And  observation  as  far  as  it  goes  supports  this 
deduction  from  anatomical  relationships.  We  will,  however,  defer 
what  else  we  have  to  say  on  this  point  until  after  we  have  discussed 
the  carrying  out  of  voluntary  movements. 


SEC.  6.     ON  SOME  HISTOLOGICAL  FEATURES  OF 
THE  BRAIN. 

§  647.  The  white  matter  of  the  brain,  as  we  have  already 
said,  like  that  of  the  spinal  cord  consists  of  medullated  fibres,  of 
various  sizes,  imbedded  in  neuroglia  and  supported  by  septa  of 
connective  tissue  derived  from  the  pia  mater.  Save  that  cells,  or 
even  groups  or  rows  of  cells,  for  the  most  part  small  cells,  about 
many  of  which  it  may  be  debated  whether  they  are  nerve  cells  or 
neuroglia  cells,  are  frequently  seen  between  the  fibres  and  bundles 
of  fibres,  the  white  matter  of  the  brain  seems  essentially  identical 
with  that  of  the  spinal  cord. 

The  grey  matter  of  the  brain  in  general  also  corresponds  to  the 
grey  matter  of  the  cord  in  consisting  of  branching  nerve  cells,  fine 
medullated  fibres  of  peculiar  nature,  non-medullated  fibres  and 
fibrils,  with  a  few  ordinary  medullated  fibres,  all  supported  in 
neuroglia. 

The  'central'  grey  matter  is  extremely  like  that  of  the  cord 
except  that  the  nervous  elements  are  imbedded  in  a  relatively 
larger  quantity  of  neuroglia.  Immediately  underneath  the  epi- 
thelium lining  the  several  ventricles  and  the  aqueduct,  the 
neuroglia  is  especially  developed,  forming  a  distinct  layer  which 
may  be  regarded  as  a  continuation  of  the  central  gelatinous 
substance  of  the  spinal  cord,  and  which  with  the  epithelium 
overlying  it  forms  what  is  known  as  the  ependyma.  The  '  nuclei ' 
of  the  cranial  nerves  are  as  we  have  seen  comparable  to  the 
groups  of  nerve  cells  in  the  spinal  cord. 

A  great  deal  of  the  grey  matter  of  the  brain  may  be  spoken  of 
as  more  'diffuse'  or  'scattered,'  more  broken  up  by  bundles  of 
fibres  than  is  the  case  in  the  spinal  cord.  The  'reticular  formation' 
of  the  bulb,  and  of  the  tegmental  region,  is  an  extreme  form  of 
this  diffuse  grey  matter.  And  even  in  such  collections  of  indu- 
bitable grey  matter  as  the  corpus  striatum,  optic  thalamus  and 
the  like,  the  pure  grey  matter,  if  we  may  use  the  term,  is  much 
more  interrupted  and  broken  up  by  conspicuous  bundles  of  white 
fibres  than  is  the  case  in  any  region  of  the  spinal  cord.  In  the 


1022  HISTOLOGY   OF   CEREBELLUM.  [BOOK  m. 

corpora  quadrigemina  too  the  grey  matter  is  broken  up  by  sheets 
or  bundles  of  white  matter. 

The  nerve  cells  of  the  several  collections  of  grey  matter  are 
not  all  alike ;  they  present  in  different  regions  differences  in  size, 
form,  and  in  other  characters.  The  cells  of  the  nucleus  caudatus, 
for  instance,  are  rather  small  and  often  round  or  spindleshaped, 
while  those  of  the  optic  thalamus  are  large,  branched  and  rich  in 
pigment.  The  cells  of  the  substantia  nigra  are  spindleshaped,  of 
moderate  size,  and  so  loaded  with  black  pigment  (in  man)  as  to 
justify  the  name ;  those  of  the  locus  caeruleus  are  very  large  and 
spherical,  with  just  so  much  pigment  as  to  give  a  bluish  tint. 
But  our  knowledge  of  the  finer  histological  details  of  the  various 
masses  of  grey  matter  is  at  present  too  imperfect  to  afford  any 
basis  whatever  for  physiological  deductions ;  and  it  will  be  hardly 
profitable  to  dwell  upon  them.  Two  regions  of  grey  matter  alone 
call  for  special  description,  the  cortex  cerebri  and  the  superficial 
grey  matter  of  the  cerebellum. 


The  superficial  grey  matter  of  the  cerebellum. 

§  648.  The  surface  of  the  cerebellum  is  increased  by  being 
folded  or  plaited  into  leaf-like  folds,  and  each  of  these  primary  folds 
is  similarly  folded  into  a  number  of  secondary,  also  leaf-like,  folds  or 
lamellae.  Each  of  these  lamellae  consists  of  a  central  core  of  white 
matter,  the  fibres  of  which  pass  inwards  to,  and  contribute  to  form 
the  central  white  matter  of  the  cerebellum,  and  of  a  superficial  layer 
of  grey  matter.  A  section  through  a  lamella  perpendicular  to  the 
surface  shews  that  the  grey  matter  consists  essentially  of  two  layers: 
a  layer  lying  next  to  the  white  matter  formed  by  densely  crowded 
small  cells,  called  the  nuclear  layer,  and  between  this  and  the  super- 
ficial pia  mater  a  much  thicker  layer  of  peculiar  nature,  called  the 
molecular  layer.  Between  these  two  layers,  and  connected  as  we 
shall  see  with  both  of  them,  lies  a  row  of  very  large  and  remark- 
able cells,  called  the  cells  of  Purkinje,  the  bodies  of  which  abut  on 
the  nuclear  layer,  and  the  long  branches  of  which  traverse  the 
molecular  layer;  these  cells  so  placed  may  be  said  to  constitute 
a  third  layer.  Before  proceeding  further,  we  may  here  remark 
that  a  section  of  the  lamella,  that  is  one  of  the  secondary  not  one 
of  the  primary  folds,  while  still  remaining  a  vertical  section  (that 
is  perpendicular  to  the  surface)  may  be  carried  through  the  lamella 
in  different  planes,  and  that  of  these  several  planes,  the  sections 
taken  in  two  of  them  are  especially  instructive,  namely,  the  one 
taken  in  what  we  may  call  the  longitudinal  plane,  passing  from 
the  top  of  the  lamella  to  its  base,  and  the  one  taken  at  right 
angles  to  the  former,  in  what  we  may  call  the  transverse  plane. 
The  nuclear  layer  and  the  molecular  layer  present  the  same  broad 
features  in  both  longitudinal  and  transverse  sections,  but  the  long 


CHAP.  IL]  THE   BRAIN.  1023 

branched  processes  of  the  cells  of  Purkinje  since  they  run  in  the 
transverse  plane  are  adequately  seen  in  transverse  sections  only, 
longitudinal  sections  shew  only  their  profiles. 

The  molecular  layer  is  of  a  peculiar  nature.  In  many  modes 
of  preparation  and  in  many  sections  it  appears  chiefly  composed  of 
a  granular  or  dotted  ground  substance ;  hence  the  name  molecular, 
as  if  it  were  an  aggregation  of  molecules.  The  dots  however  are 
sections  of  fine  fibrils,  some  of  which  are  neuroglia  fibrils  but  others 
are  undoubtedly  nervous.  The  layer  consists  in  fact  partly  of  a 
bed  of  neuroglia  and  partly  of  nervous  elements,  and  here  perhaps 
even  more  than  elsewhere  it  is  extremely  difficult  to  say  with 
regard  to  many  of  the  elements  whether  they  are  neuroglial  or 
nervous  in  nature.  A  considerable  portion  of  the  whole  area  of 
the  molecular  layer  is  taken  up  by  the  conspicuous  branched  pro- 
cesses of  the  cells  of  Purkinje;  and  scattered  about  lie  numerous 
small  cells,  some  of  which  are  neuroglia  cells,  but  some  of  which 
are  undoubtedly  nerve  cells.  The  most  conspicuous  feature  of  the 
layer  however  is  the  presence  in  large  numbers  of  the  fine  fibrils ; 
but  before  we  speak  of  these  it  will  be  desirable  to  turn  to  the 
cells  of  Purkinje  and  the  nuclear  layer. 

The  cell  of  Purkinje  possesses  a  large  (40  yu,  by  30  //.)  flask-shaped 
body,  surrounding  a  large  conspicuous  clear,  rounded,  nucleus ;  it 
has  much  the  appearance  of  a  large  ganglion  cell.  The  base  of  the 
flask  rests  on  the  nuclear  layer,  and  from  it  there  proceeds  a  single 
axis  cylinder  process  which  passing  through  the  nuclear  layer 
somewhat  obliquely,  and  in  its  passage  acquiring  a  medulla,  joins 
the  central  white  substance  as  a  medullated  fibre.  The  cells  as 
we  have  said  form  a  single  layer  only,  but  since  this  covers  the 
nuclear  layer  over  the  whole  of  the  lamella,  a  considerable  number 
of  the  fibres  of  the  white  central  matter,  though  only  a  very  small 
fraction  of  the  whole,  are  thus  derived  from  these  cells  of  Purkinje. 
The  narrowed  neck  of  the  flask  running  outward  in  the  molecular 
layer  divides  in  an  arborescent  fashion  into  a  large  number  of 
branches  which,  spreading  out  laterally  in  the  transverse  plane 
and  stretching  as  far  as  the  surface,  ramify  through  the  molecular 
layer,  and  are  eventually  lost  to  view  as  exceedingly  fine  fibrils. 
Some  observers  maintain  that  some  of  the  fine  processes  are 
continuous  with  processes  of  the  small  nerve  cells  of  the  molecular 
layer,  but  this  is  not  admitted  by  all.  In  any  case  the  fibrillar 
terminations  of  these  cells  of  Purkinje  contribute  to  the  fine  fibrils 
of  the  molecular  layer. 

The  nuclear  layer  in  ordinary  stained  specimens  has  the 
appearance  of  a  mass  of  nuclei  closely  crowded  together  in  a  bed 
of  reticular  nature ;  and  since  the  nuclei  usually  stain  deeply, 
the  layer  stands  out  in  strong  contrast  to  the  much  less  deeply 
stained  molecular  layer.  Careful  examination  with  special  modes 
of  preparation  shews  however  that  while  some  of  the  nuclei  are 
nuclei  belonging  to  neuroglia  and  blood  vessels,  the  majority 
F.  65 


1024  HISTOLOGY   OF   CEREBELLUM.  [BOOK  in. 

belong  to  small  nerve  cells  of  a  peculiar  nature.  In  these  cells 
the  nucleus  is  surrounded  by  cell  substance  which,  forming  a  thin 
layer  immediately  around  the  nucleus,  is  chiefly  disposed  as  thin 
spreading  branches,  some  of  which  end  in  a  peculiar  arborescence 
not  unlike  a  muscle  end-plate ;  these  processes  contribute  with 
the  neuroglia  to  form  the  reticular  looking  bed  spoken  of  above.  No 
process  can  be  traced  inwards  to  the  central  white  matter ;  but  one 
of  the  processes  gives  off  a  branch,  which  passing  vertically  outwards 
takes  on  the  appearance  of  a  delicate  axis  cylinder  process  and 
runs,  without  dividing,  into  the  molecular  layer  for  a  variable 
distance,  sometimes  reaching  close  to  the  surface,  but  at  last 
divides  at  right  angles  into  two  fibrils,  which  run  in  the  longitudi- 
nal plane  in  opposite  directions  for  a  considerable  distance,  and 
are  ultimately  lost  to  view.  Since  these  cells  in  the  nuclear  layer 
are  very  numerous  and  each  gives  rise  in  the  above  manner  to 
longitudinal  fibrils,  the  molecular  layer  is  traversed  by  a  multitude 
of  fibrils,  visible  as  such  in  longitudinal  sections  but  appearing  as 
dots  in  transverse  sections,  in  which  the  cells  of  Purkinje  are  best 
displayed. 

Besides  these  longitudinal  fibrils  proceeding  from  the  cells  of 
the  nuclear  layer,  special  modes  of  preparation  similarly  disclose 
numerous  transverse  as  well  as  more  or  less  oblique  fibrils.  Many  of 
these  appear  to  result  from  the  branching  of  the  small  nerve  cells 
of  the  molecular  layer,  and  some  of  those  so  arising  descend  to  the 
layer  of  the  cells  of  Purkinje  and  end  around  the  bodies  of  those 
cells  in  remarkable  nests  of  fibrils,  without  however  actually 
making  connections  with  them. 

The  medullated  fibres  of  the  central  white  matter  of  a  lamella 
pass  on  all  sides  into  the  nuclear  layer ;  or,  put  in  another  way, 
medullated  fibres  passing  out  of  the  nuclear  layer  at  all  points 
converge  to  form  the  central  white  matter.  Some  of  these  fibres 
as  we  have  seen  begin,  or  end,  in  the  cells  of  Purkinje.  None  of 
them  appear  to  join  the  cells  of  the  nuclear  layer,  and  we  have  no 
evidence  that  any  of  them  end  or  begin  in  any  way  in  the  nuclear 
layer.  A  certain  number,  however,  may  be  seen  to  pass  through 
the  nuclear  layer  and  between  the  cells  of  Purkinje  into  the 
molecular  layer,  where  losing  their  medulla  they  divide  and 
apparently  contribute  to  the  numerous  fibrils  of  the  molecular 
layer.  The  presumption  therefore  is  that  all  the  fibres  of  the 
white  matter  begin  or  end  either  in  the  cells  of  Purkinje  or  the 
fibrils  of  the  molecular  layer. 

The  superficial  grey  matter  of  the  cerebellum  then  resembles 
the  grey  matter  of  the  spinal  cord  in  so  far  as  it  consists  of  branch- 
ing nerve  cells,  nerve  fibres,  and  nerve  fibrils  embedded  in  neu- 
roglia ;  but  the  disposition  and  features  of  the  several  factors  are 
peculiar.  We  may  take  perhaps  as  the  key  of  the  structure  the 
fibrils  of  the  molecular  layer ;  this  layer  is  relatively  very  thick, 
about  400  /z,  much  thicker  than  the  nuclear  layer  which,  however, 


CHAP.  IL]  THE   BRAIN.  1025 

varies  in  thickness,  being  generally  thickest  at  the  top  of  the 
fold ;  hence  the  number  of  fibrils  in  it  may  be  spoken  of  as 
enormous.  These  fibrils  seem  certainly  to  be  connected  on  the 
one  hand  with  the  cells  of  the  nuclear  layer  and  on  the  other 
hand  with  the  scattered  small  cells  of  their  own  layer;  but  we 
have  no  evidence  that  these  two  sets  of  fibrils  are  continuous  with 
each  other ;  on  the  contrary,  it  seems  more  prpbable  that  the  two 
sets  of  cells  represent  two  independent  systems.  We  can  hardly 
doubt  that  these  fibrils  are  in  functional  connection  with  the  medul- 
lated  fibres  of  the  central  white  matter;  but  we  have  no  clear 
evidence  that  the  system  of  scattered  cells  is  continuous  either  with 
the  cells  of  Purkinje,  and  so  with  the  medullated  fibres  belonging  to 
those  cells,  or  with  the  medullated  fibres  which  end  independently 
in  the  molecular  layer ;  and  we  have  no  evidence  at  all  that  the 
system  of  the  cells  of  the  nuclear  layer  is  connected  with  either. 
We  can  hardly  think  otherwise  than  that  the  molecular  changes 
which  sweep  to  and  fro  along  the  tangle  of  these  fibrils  (whose 
nutrition  is  probably  governed  and  hence  whose  functional  activity 
is  probably  regulated  by  the  nuclear  and  scattered  cells  respec- 
tively) are  influenced  by  or  originate  the  nervous  impulses  passing 
along  the  medullated  fibres  of  the  white  matter;  and  hence  we 
must  conclude  that  either  a  continuity  exists  which  has  as  yet 
escaped  detection  or,  what  is  quite  possible  if  not  probable,  that 
one  fibril  can  act  upon  another  by  simple  contact  or  even  at  a  dis- 
tance. Further,  while  the  cell  of  Purkinje,  with  its  large  cell  body 
and  nucleus,  its  conspicuous  axis  cylinder  process  and  its  other 
branched  processes  presents  many  analogies  with  a  motor  cell, 
such  as  those  of  the  anterior  horn  of  the  spinal  cord,  and  raises 
the  presumption  that  the  impulses  which  move  along  its  axis- 
cylinder  process,  proceed  outwards  from  the  cell  as  motor  or  at 
least  as  efferent  impulses,  we  have  no  direct  proof  that  this  is  so. 
And  though  it  is  tempting  to  suppose  that  the  other  medullated 
fibres,  which  like  the  fibres  of  a  posterior  root  are  lost  in  the  grey 
matter,  without  the  intervention  of  a  conspicuous  cell,  carry 
afferent  impulses,  we  have  as  yet  no  proof  of  this.  All  we  can  say 
is  that  the  grey  matter  is  connected  in  two  different  ways  with  at 
least  two  sets  of  fibres,  which  probably  therefore  have  different 
functions. 

We  may  here  add  the  remark  that  the  large  body  of  the  cell 
of  Purkinje  lies,  as  indeed  do  the  other  nervous  elements,  in  an 
appropriate  space  in  the  bed  of  neuroglia.  Between  the  surface 
of  the  cell  and  the  wall  of  neuroglia  is  a  space,  generally  so 
narrow  as  to  be  potential  rather  than  actual,  but  which  may 
sometimes  be  considerable.  Whether  small  or  large  it  contains 
lymph,  and  the  cavity  in  which  the  cell  lies  is  in  connection 
with  the  lymphatics  of  the  brain.  Each  cell  then  lies  in  a 
lymph  space ;  but  we  merely  mention  the  fact  now ;  we  shall 
have  to  return  to  the  matter  when  we  come  to  deal  with  the 

65—2 


1026  HISTOLOGY   OF   CORTEX.  [BOOK  in. 

lymphatic  and  vascular   arrangements   of  the   brain   and  spinal 
cord. 


The  Cerebral  Cortex. 

§  649.  While  the  superficial  grey  matter  of  the  cerebellum 
does  not  differ  strikingly  as  to  its  histological  features  in  different 
regions,  very  considerable  differences  are  observed  in  different 
regions  of  the  cerebral  cortex.  A  general  plan  of  structure  may 
perhaps  be  recognized,  but  as  we  pass  from  one  part  of  the  cerebral 
surface  to  another  we  find  modifications  continually  taking  place. 
We  must  content  ourselves  here  with  attempting  a  description  of 
the  general  plan  followed  by  an  indication  of  the  more  striking 
characteristics  of  certain  regions. 

The  cortical  grey  matter,  having  an  average  thickness  of  about 
3  mm.,  but  varying  considerably  in  different  regions  from  1*8  mm. 
in  some  parts  of  the  occipital  lobe  to  4'2  at  the  dorsal  summit 
of  the  precentral  convolution,  is,  like  other  grey  matter,  composed 
of  nerve  cells,  and  of  nerve  fibres  and  fibrils  supported  by  neu- 
roglia.  The  nerve  cells,  at  least  the  conspicuous  and  easily 
recognized  nerve  cells,  are  scattered,  and  appear,  in  sections, 
to  be  imbedded  in,  and  separated  from  each  other  by  a  not  in- 
considerable but  variable  quantity  of  somewhat  peculiar  ground 
substance,  not  unlike  that  which  forms  so  large  a  part  of  the 
molecular  layer  of  the  cerebellum.  Part  of  this  ground  substance, 
which  apparently  is  not  confined  to  any  particular  layer,  but 
stretches  throughout  the  thickness  of  the  cortex  is  undoubtedly 
neuroglial  in  nature,  but  part,  and  probably  the  greater  part,  is 
nervous  in  nature ;  it  is  largely  composed  of  fine  fibrils  traversing 
it  in  various  directions,  the  transverse  sections  of  these  fibrils  giving 
it  a  characteristic  dotted  or  '  molecular  '  appearance  ;  and  the  ma- 
jority of  these  fine  fibrils  are  probably  the  continuations  of 
branching  nerve  cells  or  dividing  nerve  fibres,  the  remainder  being 
neuroglial  fibrils.  In  this  respect  it  resembles  the  molecular  layer 
of  the  cerebellum,  but  it  is,  to  a  much  greater  extent  than  is  that 
layer,  traversed  by  medullated  nerve  fibres,  especially  by  fine  me- 
dullated  fibres  like  those  seen  in  the  grey  matter  of  the  spinal 
cord,  §  563. 

The  nerve  cells  imbedded  in  this  ground  substance  in  more  or 
less  distinct  layers  are  of  various  kinds.  The  most  conspicuous, 
abundant  and  characteristic  nerve  cells  found  in  the  cortex  of 
all  regions  of  the  cerebrum,  are  those  which  from  their  shape 
are  called  pyramidal  cells.  These  vary  much  in  size  and  have 
been  distinguished  as  'small  pyramidal'  cells  averaging  12  p  in 
length  by  8  p  in  breadth,  and  '  large  pyramidal '  cells,  sometimes 
called  '  ganglionic  cells/  of  which  the  medium  size  is  about  40  //, 
in  length  by  20  p  in  breadth.  Some  of  the  latter,  occurring  in 


CHAP.  IL]  THE   BRAIN.  1027 

special  regions  are  of  very  large  size,   120  /-t  by  50  /&  and  have 
been  called  'giant  cells/ 

The  features  of  a  '  large  pyramidal '  cell  are  very  characteristic. 
Such  a  cell  appears  in  a  well  prepared  vertical  section  of  the 
cortex  as  an  elongated  isosceles  triangle  placed  vertically,  with 
the  base  looking  towards  the  underlying  white  substance  and  the 
tapering  apex  pointing  to  the  surface.  The  cell  substance  is 
finely  granulated  or  fibrillated,  the  fibrillae  sweeping  round  in 
various  directions;  it  not  unfrequently  contains  pigment.  In 
the  midst  of  this  cell-substance  rather  near  the  base  lies  a  large 
clear  conspicuous  round  or  oval  nucleolated  nucleus.  At  the  base 
the  cell-substance  is  prolonged  into  a  number  of  processes.  One 
of  these,  generally  starting  from  about  the  middle  of  the  base, 
runs  for  some  distance  without  dividing,  and  soon  acquiring  a 
medulla  may  be  recognized  as  an  axis  cylinder  process ;  the  fibre 
to  which  it  gives  origin  sweeps  with  a  more  or  less  curved  course 
into  the  subjacent  white  matter.  In  some  instances  the  axis 
cylinder  process,  by  a  T  division  like  that  seen  in  a  ganglion  of  a 
posterior  root  (§97)  gives  rise  to  two  fibres,  one  of  which  may 
take  a  horizontal  direction  ;  in  some  regions  of  the  cortex,  the 
occipital  for  instance,  the  axis  cylinder  process  is  said  to  give  rise 
by  division  to  several  fibres.  The  other  processes  from  the  base, 
especially  those  from  the  angles  of  the  triangle,  rapidly  branch 
into  fine  fibrils  which  are  soon  lost  to  view  in  the  ground 
substance.  The  apex  of  the  triangle  is  also  prolonged  into  a 
process,  which  giving  off  fine  lateral  branches,  makes  as  it  were 
straight  for  the  surface,  but  ultimately  branching  into  fine  fibrils 
is  lost  to  view  at  some  distance  from  the  body  of  the  cell.  The 
cell  lies  in  a  cavity  of  the  ground  substance  which  it  appears 
normally  to  fill,  but  from  the  walls  of  which  it  sometimes  shrinks, 
developing  between  itself  and  the  wall  of  the  cavity  a  space  which 
may  contain  not  only  lymph  but  occasionally  leucocytes.  In 
prepared  specimens  the  retraction  within  its  cavity  of  the  arti- 
ficially shrunken  cell  may  be  often  observed. 

The  'small  pyramidal'  cells  have  much  the  same  features;' 
that  is  to  say  the  cells  are  characterized  by  their  pyramidal 
form,  though  this  is  naturally  not  so  distinct,  by  their  vertical 
position,  and  by  the  possession  of  branching  processes  which  are 
lost  in  the  molecular  ground  substance ;  the  presence  however  of  a 
midbasal  axis- cylinder  process  has  not  been  clearly  demonstrated. 

Other  nerve  cells  are  more  like  the  ordinary  nerve  cells  of  the 
spinal  cord  and  of  the  internal  cerebral  grey  matter ;  they  are 
branched  cells  of  irregular,  not  of  pyramidal  form  and  for  the 
most  part  small,  18  p  by  10  yu,.  They  may  be  characterized  by 
the  relative  large  size  (7  /A)  of  the  nucleus,  and  do  not  possess 
an  axis  cylinder  process ;  at  least  such  a  process  has  not  yet 
been  demonstrated.  They  are  frequently  spoken  of  as  '  angular ' 
cells. 


1028  HISTOLOGY   OF   CORTEX.  [BOOK  m. 

Another  kind  of  cell,  the  '  fusiform  cell,'  which  is  found  in 
all  regions  of  the  cortex  has  a  characteristic  spindle  shape,  the 
cell-substance  being  prolonged  at  the  opposite  poles  into  tapering, 
ultimately  branched  processes.  The  long  axis  of  the  cell  is  gene- 
rally placed  horizontally,  following  the  curvature  of  the  cortex, 
and  being  thus  at  the  sides  of  the  sulci  vertical  to  the  surface 
of  the  brain  ;  it  is  however  at  times  inclined  at  various  angles. 

Still  another  kind  of  cell,  the  '  granule  cell '  or  '  nuclear 
cell,'  is  one  in  which  the  nucleus  is  surrounded  by  a  relatively 
small  quantity  of  cell  substance,  9  yu.  by  7  JJL,  more  or  less  spherical 
in  form  in  ordinary  preparations,  but  probably  breaking  up  into 
delicate  branched  processes.  Cells  of  this  kind  are  sparsely  scat- 
tered throughout  the  cortex  generally,  but  in  particular  regions, 
e.g.  the  occipital,  are  crowded  together  into  a  layer,  which  in 
many  respects  resembles  the  nuclear  layer  of  the  cerebellum,  arid 
has  been  called  the  '  granular '  or  '  nuclear '  layer. 

Lastly  throughout  the  cortex  are  found  besides  indubitable 
nerve  cells  and  indubitable  neuroglial  cells,  numerous  small  some- 
what irregular  cells,  concerning  which  it  may  be  debated  whether 
they  are  really  nervous  or  simply  neuroglial  in  nature.  Moreover 
in  using  the  names  given  above  for  the  various  kinds  of  nerve  cells, 
it  must  be  remembered  that  many  transitional  forms  are  observed; 
cells  for  instance  may  be  seen  intermediate  in  form  between  pyra- 
midal cells  and  '  fusiform '  or  '  angular '  cells. 

The  medullated  nerve  fibres  which  take  part  in  the  cortex  may 
be  considered  provisionally  as  forming  two  categories.  In  the 
first  place  fibres  sweep  up  vertically  into  the  cortex  from  the 
subjacent  '  central  white  matter '  taking  at  first  a  curved  course 
as  they  enter  into  the  grey  matter  and  then  appearing  to  run 
straight  towards  the  surface.  These  are  arranged  in  the  deeper 
levels  in  bundles,  leaving  vertical  columns  of  the  grey  matter 
between  them ;  but  at  more  superficial  levels  the  bundles  spread 
out  and  are  gradually  lost  to  view.  Besides  these  distinct 
vertical  fibres  and  bundles  of  fibres,  of  the  ordinary  medullated 
kind,  which  we  have  reason  to  think  are  the  ends  (or  beginnings) 
on  the  one  hand  of  fibres  of  the  pedal  and  tegmental  systems  and 
on  the  other  hand  of  fibres  of  the  corpus  callosum,  or  the  other 
commissural  fibres  spoken  of  as  '  association  '  fibres  (§  635),  an 
exceedingly  large  number  of  fibres  of  the  peculiar  fine  medullated 
kind  run  in  various  directions,  forming  a  dense  network  in  the 
ground  substance  of  the  grey  matter  between  the  cells.  We  may 
add  that  this  system  of  fine  medullated  fibres  is  of  late  growth 
and  is  not  fully  developed  in  man  until  two  or  three  years  after 
birth.  Many  of  the  medullated  fibres,  coarse  as  well  as  fine,  take  a 
horizontal  direction  parallel  to  the  surface,  and  in  certain  regions 
are  specially  developed  into  a  layer  or  into  two  layers  so  as  to 
form  a  horizontal  streak  or  streaks. 

The  vascular  pia  mater  invests  closely  as  we  have  said  the 


CHAP,  ii.]  THE   BRAIN.  1029 

whole  surface  of  the  cortex,  dipping  down  into  'the  sulci ;  and 
from  it,  as  in  the  case  of  the  spinal  cord,  processes  carrying  blood 
vessels  and  bearing  lymph  spaces  pass  inwards  to  supply  the  grey 
matter  with  blood.  But  while,  as  we  shall  see  later  on,  the  supply 
of  blood  vessels  to  the  grey  matter  is  considerable,  the  truly 
connective  tissue  elements  of  the  pia  mater  processes  are  soon 
merged  into  neuroglia.  Immediately  beneath  the  pia  mater 
forming  the  immediate  surface  of  the  cortex  is  a  thin  layer 
consisting  of  neuroglia  only. 

§  650.  The  nerve  cells  of  the  above  several  kinds  are  arranged 
more  or  less  distinctly  in  layers  parallel  to  the  surface,  so  that  the 
whole  thickness  of  the  cortex  may  by  means  of  them  be,  more  or 
less  successfully,  divided  into  a  series  of  zones,  one  above  the  other; 
and  we  may  as  we  have  said  recognize  on  the  one  hand  a  general 
arrangement  common  to  the  whole  surface,  and  on  the  other  hand 
modifications  existing  in  the  several  regions.  The  general 
arrangement  may  be  said  to  be  one  of  five  layers  or  zones,  usually 
counted  from  the  surface  inwards. 

The  fifth  layer,  lying  next  to  the  central  white  matter,  fairly 
uniform  in  characters  and  thickness  (about  1  mm.)  over  the 
greater  part  of  the  brain  is  characterized  by  the  presence  of  some- 
what sparsely  scattered  '  fusiform  '  cells,  though  other  branched 
cells  are  present.  It  is  broken  up  into  vertical  columns  by  the 
bundles  of  vertical  fibres,  and  its  demarcation  from  the  white 
matter  below  is  somewhat  indistinct  owing  to  the  fact  that  in  the 
brain  the  white  matter,  especially  that  lying  beneath  the  cortex, 
contains  cells  and  small  groups  of  cells  lying  between  the  bundles 
of  fibres  to  a  much  greater  extent  than  does  the  white  matter  of 
the  spinal  cord. 

The  fourth  layer,  lying  above  the  preceding,  varies  much  more 
both  in  thickness  ('35  mm.  to  '15  mm.)  and  in  its  characters. 
The  constituent  cells  are  on  the  one  hand  large  pyramidal  cells, 
and  on  the  other  hand  'granule'  or  'nuclear'  cells.  In  some 
regions  it  may  be  subdivided  into  two  layers,  the  small  'nuclear' 
cells  being  so  abundant  as  to  form  in  the  upper  part  of  the  layer 
a  separate  layer  called  the  '  granule '  or  '  nuclear '  layer.  This 
fourth  layer  like  the  preceding  fifth  layer  beneath  it  is  split  up 
into  vertical  columns  by  the  bundles  of  vertical  fibres,  but  to  a 
less  degree.  It  is  marked  in  its  lower  part  by  a  horizontal  streak 
due  to  numerous,  mostly  fine,  medullated  fibres  running  horizon- 
tally. In  the  cortex  of  the  Island  of  Heil,  this  horizontal  layer  is 
developed  into  a  conspicuous  sheet  of  medullated  fibres,  separating 
the  fourth  and  fifth  layers  by  a  distinct  interval  of  obvious  white 
matter.  This  fifth  layer,  of  fusiform  cells,  thus  detached  from  the 
rest  of  the  cortex  is  what  is  called  the  claustrum  (Figs.  115,  116, 
&c.,  cl). 

In  the  third  layer,  the  constituent  cells  are  the  characteristic 
pyramidal  cells.  These  are  for  the  most  part  large,  though 


1030  HISTOLOGY   OF   CORTEX.  [BOOK  in. 

diminishing  in  'size  from  below  upwards,  and  the  layer  has  been 
called  the  "layer  of  large  pyramidal  cells,"  though  in  certain 
regions  the  largest  pyramidal  cells,  and  notably  the  giant  cells  are 
found  in  the  preceding,  fourth,  layer.  The  cells  are  on  the  whole 
scattered  somewhat  sparsely,  though  frequently  gathered  into 
small  groups,  and  among  them  occur  small  '  nuclear '  and  other 
cells.  The  bundles  of  vertical  fibres  spread  out  rapidly  in  this 
layer  so  that  the  columnar  arrangement  becomes  lost,  and  many 
of  the  fibres  undoubtedly  become  axis  cylinder  processes  of  the 
pyramidal  cells.  Though  the  layer  varies  in  thickness  (1  mm. 
to  *4  mm.)  and  in  some  of  its  features  in  different  regions,  the 
characteristic  pyramidal  cells  are  present  over  the  whole  surface 
of  the  hemisphere.  In  the  lower  part  of  the  layer  a  second 
horizontal  streak  of  closely  interwoven  horizontal  fibres  frequently 
makes  its  appearance. 

The  second  layer,  generally  a  thin  one,  though  varying  from 
'25  mm.  to  '75  mm.  in  thickness,  is  also  formed  by  pyramidal  cells 
but  is  distinguished  from  the  layer  below  by  the  absence  of  large 
and  medium  sized  cells  and  by  the  presence  of  numerous  small 
cells  closely  packed  together;  it  has  been  called  "the  layer  of 
small  pyramidal  cells."  As  we  have  said  these  smaller  pyramidal 
cells  differ  somewhat  from  the  larger  cells ;  and  the  cells  in  this 
layer  are  sometimes  described  as  '  angular.' 

The  first  and  most  superficial  layer  is  characterized  by  the 
predominance  of  the  molecular  ground  substance,  the  cells  being 
few,  far  between,  small,  and  irregular.  The  ground  substance 
itself  seems  to  be  more  largely  neuroglial  in  nature  than  in 
the  other  layers,  and,  as  we  said  above,  its  extreme  surface 
appears  to  be  furnished  by  neuroglia  alone.  The  layer  is  gener- 
ally spoken  of  as  the  '  peripheral '  or  '  superficial  layer,'  or  some- 
times as  the  'molecular'  layer.  The  tapering  vertical  processes 
of  the  pyramidal  cells  may  be  traced  into  this  layer,  which  indeed 
varies  in  thickness  according  to  the  abundance  of  pyramidal  cells 
in  the  subjacent  layers;  numerous  somewhat  fine  medullated  fibres 
also  traverse  it  in  a  horizontal  direction. 

§  651.  The  general  arrangement  just  described  varies  as  we 
have  said  in  different  regions  of  the  cerebral  surface.  We  must 
content  ourselves  here  with  pointing  out  the  characteristics  of 
two  or  three  important  regions. 

The  region  which  we  have  (§  632)  called  the  '  motor  area '  or 
'  region,'  is  characterized  on  the  one  hand  by  the  great  thickness 
(1  mm.)  of  the  third  layer,  that  of  large  pyramidal  cells,  as  well  as 
by  the  number  and  size  of  the  cells  contained  in  it,  and  on  the 
other  hand  and  especially,  by  the  prominence  in  the  fourth  layer 
of  remarkable  clusters  of  very  large  pyramidal  cells,  of  the  kind 
which  are  referred  to  above,  §  649,  as  being  frequently  called 
'  ganglionic ' ;  it  is  in  this  region  that  '  giant  cells '  are  found  in 
the  fourth  layer,  namely,  in  the  upper  part  of  the  precentral  and 


CHAP,  ii.]  THE   BRAIN.  1031 

at  the  summit  of  the  postcentral  convolution,  and  in  the  para- 
ceritral  lobule,  acquiring  their  greatest  size  at  the  top  of  the 
precentral  convolution. 

The  occipital  region  is  characterized  by  the  prominence  of  the 
'  granule '  or  '  nuclear '  cells.  These  not  only  form  a  distinct 
division  of  the  fourth  layer,  but  are  also  conspicuous  in  other 
layers,  their  arrangement  being  such  that  some  authors  have 
been  led  to  divide  the  cortex  of  this  region  into  seven  or  even 
eight  layers.  In  the  present  state  of  our  knowledge  we  may 
be  content  with  insisting  that  the  great  mark  of  this  occipital 
region  is  the  abundance  of  these  small  'nuclear'  cells  together 
with  other  small  '  angular '  cells,  whereby  the  pyramidal  cells  seem 
to  be  made  less  conspicuous.  It  is  worthy  of  notice  however 
that  in  the  third,  but  more  especially  in  the  fourth  layer,  a 
few  cells  of  very  large  size  are  met  with,  which  by  their  large 
branched  cell  substance  and  conspicuous  axis  cylinder  process 
resemble  the  large  cells  in  the  motor  region  ;  but  it  should  be 
noted  that  while  these  large  cells  occur,  (at  least  in  man  and  in 
the  monkey,  though  not  in  some  of  the  lower  animals  as  the 
rabbit),  in  very  definite  clusters  in  the  motor  region,  they  occur 
singly  in  the  occipital  region.  In  this  occipital  region  the  layer 
of  horizontal  fibres  in  the  fourth  layer  is  very  conspicuous,  and 
owing  to  the  number  of  ordinary  medullated  fibres  present  forms 
a  white  streak  visible  even  to  the  naked  eye. 

In  the  frontal  region,  in  front  of  the  motor  region,  the  arrange- 
ment is  more  in  accordance  with  what  we  have  described  as  the 
general  plan.  The  two  pyramidal  layers  are  well  marked  as  is  alsa 
the  fourth  layer;  but  the  layer  of  large  pyramidal  cells  is  much 
thinner  than  in  the  motor  region,  as  is  also,  though  to  a  less 
extent,  the  fourth  layer,  while  the  fifth  layer,  that  of  fusiform  cells, 
is  thicker  than  elsewhere.  Small  '  nuclear '  cells  are  perhaps 
more  abundant  in  this  region  throughout  all  layers  than  in  the 
motor  region,  but  are  far  less  conspicuous  than  in  the  occipital 
region. 

We  may  here  remark  that  the  transition  in  structure  from  one 
region  to  another  is  very  gradual,  not  sharp  and  distinct,  and  is 
perhaps  especially  gradual  in  passing  from  the  motor  region 
backwards  to  the  occipital  region.  It  is  not  possible  to  recognize 
histologically  the  limit,  for  instance,  of  the  motor  region  as 
determined  experimentally. 

In  special  regions  of  the  brain,  for  instance  in  the  olfactory 
bulb  of  which  we  shall  speak  later  on,  very  great  modifications 
of  the  general  plan  may  be  observed  in  the  cortex.  We  cannot 
enter  upon  these  but  may  just  refer  to  the  cornu  ammonis  or 
hippocampus.  At  the  ventral  end  of  the  temporal  lobe,  the  gyrus 
hippocampi,  the  structure  of  whose  cortex  follows  the  general  plan, 
is  thrust  inward  so  as  to  project  into  the  cavity  of  the  descending 
horn  of  the  lateral  ventricle,  forming  the  ridge-like  prominence 


1032  HISTOLOGY   OF   CORTEX.  [BOOK  in. 

known  by  the  above  name.  The  substance  of  the  cornu  ammonis 
is  therefore  cortical  substance  covered  on  the  side  of  the  ventricle 
by  a  thin  prolongation  of  the  central  white  matter  which  is  in 
turn  covered  by  the  ependyma  lining  the  ventricle.  A  vertical 
section  of  this  substance  shews  that  while  the  fifth  and  fourth 
layers  are  reduced  to  small  dimensions,  the  third  layer,  that  of 
large  pyramidal  cells,  is  well  developed  though  narrow.  The  cells 
are  large  and  remarkably  long,  and  the  tapering  processes  are 
arranged  so  regularly  as  to  give  rise  especially  in  stained  pre- 
parations to  a  marked  radiate  appearance.  At  the  level  of  the 
second  layer  there  occurs  a  large  development  of  capillary  blood 
vessels  and  a  scarceness  of  cells,  giving  rise  to  a  '  lacunar ' 
appearance ;  and  the  first  or  molecular  layer  is  of  some  con- 
siderable thickness.  From  the  prominence  of  the  pyramidal  cells 
in  this  region,  the  third  layer  in  the  general  plan  of  the  cortex 
has  sometimes  been  spoken  of  as  the  "  formation  of  the  cornu 
ammonis." 

§  652.  In  the  present  state  of  knowledge  it  is  impossible  to 
come  to  any  satisfactory  conclusion  concerning  the  meaning  of  the 
variety  and  arrangement  of  the  cells  and  other  constituents  of  the 
cortex.  The  cells  with  their  branches,  the  nerve  fibres  and  the 
nerve  fibrils  form  a  network  of  grey  matter  which  we  may  compare 
with  the  grey  matter  of  the  spinal  cord  (§  579)  but  which  is 
obviously,  as  we  might  expect,  far  more  complex  than  that  is. 
We  may  conclude,  and  experimental  observation  confirms  the 
conclusion,  that  the  large  pyramidal  cells  with  recognisable  axis 
cylinder  processes  serve  as  trophic  centres  for  the  fibres  which 
appear  to  start  from  them.  And  we  may,  though  with  less 
confidence,  explain  the  large  size  of  these  cells  in  the  motor 
region,  by  the  fact  that  they  give  rise  to  fibres  of  the  pyramidal 
tract  stretching  a  long  way  from  their  origin  in  the  cell,  and 
therefore  demanding  great  nutritive  activity  on  the  part  of  the 
cell.  We  may  perhaps  also  conclude  that  these  fibres  are  efferent, 
motor  fibres,  destined  to  carry  impulses  from  the  cortex  to  peri- 
pheral or  at  least  distant  parts.  And  we  may  further,  with 
however  distinctly  less  confidence,  assume  that  the  size  of  the  cell 
is  correlated  to  the  energy  which  has  to  be  expended  in  the 
discharge  of  efferent,  motor  impulses.  If  we  accept  these 
conclusions  we  must  also  bear  in  mind,  that  such  cells,  with 
axis  cylinder  processes  continued  on  as  fibres,  are  not  limited  to, 
though  most  abundant  in  the  motor  region,  but  are  found  in  all 
regions  of  the  cortex ;  and  we  must  hence  conclude  that  im- 
pulses, which  we  must  call  efferent,  proceed  from  all  parts  of  the 
cortex. 

It  is  obvious  however  that  the  connection  of  the  cortical  net- 
work of  grey  matter  with  the  fibres  of  the  white  matter  is  effected 
in  part  only,  and  that  a  small  part,  by  the  method  of  axis-cylinder 
processes  definitely  prolonged  from  the  cell  substance  of  cells.  A 


CHAP,  ii.]  THE   BRAIN.  1033 

part,  and  probably  a  greater  part  of  the  fibres  sweeping  up  from 
the  subjacent  white  matter,  whether  they  be  fibres  of  the  pedal 
and  tegmental  systems  or  callosal  or  'association'  fibres,  end  in 
the  grey  matter  in  some  other  way  than  by  bodily  being  continued 
into  the  cell  substance  of  cells ;  they  plunge  into  and  break  up 
within  the  network,  of  which  fibrils  no  less  than  cells  form  a 
conspicuous  part ;  and  we  may  here  repeat  the  remark  which 
we  made  in  speaking  of  the  cerebellum  concerning  the  actual 
continuity  of  the  elements  of  the  network.  Moreover,  besides  the 
vertical  fibres  obviously  coming  from  the  subjacent  white  matter, 
we  have  in  this  grey  matter  to  deal  with  the  fibres  of  horizontal 
and  other  directions,  which  may  come  from  white  matter,  not  far 
off,  but  which  may  come  from  some  neighbouring  grey  matter ; 
our  present  knowledge  will  not  enable  us  to  settle  this  point. 

In  the  spinal  cord  we  were  able  to  divide  all  the  fibres  into 
afferent  and  efferent  respectively ;  though  even  here  we  met  with 
some  difficulty.  Dealing  with  the  cerebral  cortex,  which  as  we 
have  already  seen  is  certainly  especially  concerned  in  voluntary 
movements  and  in  the  development  of  full  sensations,  we  may  be 
tempted  to  consider  the  fibres  connected  with  the  grey  matter  as 
similarly  divisible  into  motor  and  sensory ;  and  we  may  go  on  to 
suppose  that  the  fibres  joining  the  cortex  as  axis  cylinder  pro- 
cesses of  recognisable  cells  are  motor  fibres,  and  that  all  the  other 
fibres  joining  the  grey  matter  in  some  other  way  are  sensory  fibres. 
But  in  doing  so  we  are  going  beyond  our  tether ;  in  all  probability 
the  nervous  processes  going  on  in  the  cortex  are  far  too  complex  to 
permit  such  a  simple  classification  of  the  functions  of  fibres  as  that 
into  motor  and  sensory ;  and  any  attempt  to  arrange  either  fibres 
or  regions  of  the  cortex  as  simply  motor  or  sensory  is  probably 
misleading.  But  we  shall  have  to  return  to  these  matters  when 
we  deal  with  the  functions  of  the  cortex. 


SEC.  7.     ON   VOLUNTARY   MOVEMENTS. 


§  653.  When  we  examine  ourselves  we  recognize  certain  of  our 
movements  as  'voluntary';  we  say  that  we  carry  them  out  by  an 
effort  of  the  '  will.'  And  when  we  witness  the  movements  of  other 
people  or  of  animals  we  regard  as  also  voluntary  such  of  those 
movements  as  by  their  characters  and  by  the  circumstances  of 
their  occurrence  seem  to  be  carried  out  in  the  same  way  as  our 
own  voluntary  movements.  Even  in  the  case  of  some  of  our  own 
movements  we  are  not  always  clear  whether  they  are  really  volun- 
tary or  no ;  and  in  the  case  of  other  people  and  of  animals  it  is 
still  more  difficult  to  decide  the  question.  It  would  be  out  of 
place  to  attempt  to  discuss  here  how  voluntary  movements  really 
differ  from  involuntary  movements,  or  in  other  words,  what  is  the 
nature  of  the  will ;  we  must  be  content  to  take  a  somewhat  rough 
use  of  the  words  '  voluntary,'  '  volitional,'  and  '  will '  as  a  basis  for 
physiological  discussion.  We  may  however  remark  that  as  far  as 
the  muscular  side  of  the  act,  if  we  may  use  such  an  expression,  is 
concerned,  a  voluntary  movement  does  not  differ  in  kind  from  an 
involuntary  movement.  It  is  perfectly  true  that  a  skilled  man 
may  by  practice  learn  to  execute  muscular  manoeuvres  which 
he  would  not  have  learnt  to  execute  had  not  an  intelligent  volition 
been  operative  within  him ;  but  our  own  experience  teaches  us  that 
many  more  or  less  intricate  movements  which  have  undoubtedly 
been  learnt  by  help  of  the  will  may  be  carried  out  under  circum- 
stances of  such  a  kind  that  we  feel  compelled  to  regard  them  as,  at 
the  time,  involuntary;  and  it  may  at  least  be  debated  whether 
every  movement  which  we  can  carry  out,  by  an  effort  of  the  will, 
may  not  appear  under  appropriate  circumstances  as  part  of  an  in- 
voluntary act.  In  the  case  of  the  lower  animals,  in  the  frog  deprived 
of  its  cerebral  hemispheres  for  instance,  we  have  seen  that  volun- 
tary differ  from  involuntary  movements,  not  by  their  essential 
nature  but  by  the  relation  which  their  occurrence  bears  to 
circumstances.  We  have  therefore  to  seek  for  the  distinction 
between  voluntary  and  involuntary,  not  in  the  coordination  of  the 
muscular  and  nervous  components  of  a  movement,  but  in  the 
nature  of  the  process  which  starts  the  whole  act. 


CHAP,  ii.]  THE   BRAIN.      *  1035 

The  histories,  related  in  a  preceding  section,  of  various  animals 
deprived  of  their  cerebral  hemispheres,  while  they  have  further 
shewn  the  difficulty  of  drawing  a  sharp  line  between  the  presence 
and  absence  of  volition,  such  as  when  we  appeal  to  our  own 
consciousness  we  seem  able  to  draw,  have  taught  us  that  in  a 
broad  sense  the  presence  of  volition  is,  in  the  higher  vertebrata, 
dependent  on  the  possession  of  the  cerebral  hemispheres ;  and  we 
have  now  to  inquire  what  we  know  concerning  the  way  in  which 
the  cerebral  cortex,  for  this,  as  we  have  seen,  is  the  important 
part  of  the  cerebral  hemisphere,  by  the  help  of  other  parts  of  the 
nervous  system  carries  out  a  voluntary  movement. 

§  654.  With  this  view  we  may  at  once  turn  to  the  results  of 
experimental  interference  with  the  cortex.  When  the  surface  of 
the  brain  is  laid  bare  by  removal  of  the  skull  and  dura  mater, 
mechanical  stimulation  of  the  cortex  produces  little  or  no  effect, 
thus  affording  a  contrast  with  the  results  of  mechanically  stimu- 
lating other  portions  of  the  brain,  or  other  nervous  structures. 
And  for  a  long  time  the  cortex  was  spoken  of  as  insensible  to 
stimulation.  When,  however,  the  electric  current  is  employed, 
either  the  make  and  break  of  the  constant  current,  or  the  more 
manageable  interrupted  current,  very  marked  results  follow.  It  is 
found  that  certain  movements  follow  upon  electric  stimulation  of 
certain  regions  or  areas.  The  results,  moreover,  differ  in  different 
animals.  It  will  be  convenient  to  begin  with  the  dog,  on  which 
animal  the  observations  of  this  kind  were  first  conducted. 

When  the  surface  of  the  dog's  brain  is  viewed  from  the  dorsal 
surface  a  short  but  deep  sulcus  is  seen  towards  the  front,  running 
outwards  almost  at  right  angles  from  the  great  longitudinal 
fissure  ;  this  is  called  the  crucial  sulcus  (Fig.  124),  the  gyrus  or 
convolution  in  front  and  behind  it,  and  sweeping  round  its  end 
being  called  the  sigmoid  gyrus.  It  will  hardly  be  profitable  to 
discuss  here  either  the  homology  of  this  sulcus  or  the  names  of 
the  other  sulci  and  convolutions  of  the  dog's  brain.  We  mention 
this  sulcus  because  it  is  found  that  stimulation  of  the  cortex  in  a 
region  which  may  be  broadly  described  as  that  of  the  neighbourhood 
of  this  crucial  sulcus  gives  rise  to  movements  of  various  parts  of 
the  body,  whereas  no  such  movements  result  from  stimulation  of 
the  extreme  frontal  region  in  front  of  the  area  around  the  crucial 
sulcus,  or  from  stimulation  of  the  occipital  region  behind  this 
area.  Certain  exceptions  may  be  made  to  this  broad  statement, 
but  these  it  will  be  best  to  discuss  in  reference  to  the  more 
highly  developed  monkey. 

The  region  of  the  cortex  in  the  neighbourhood  of  the  crucial 
sulcus  may  then  be  termed  an  '  excitable '  or  '  motor '  region,  inas- 
much as  stimulation  of  this  region  leads  to  movements  carried  out 
by  skeletal  muscles,  while  stimulation  of  other  regions  does  not. 
Further,  stimulation  of  particular  districts  or  areas  of  the  region 
leads  to  particular  movements  carried  out  by  particular  muscles. 


1036  CORTICAL   MOTOR   REGION.  [BOOK  in. 

For  instance,  stimulation  of  the  more  median  parts  of  the  gyrus 
behind  the  crucial  sulcus  (Fig.  124  JJ)  leads  to  movements  of  the 
hind  limb,  whereas  stimulation  of  the  lateral  part  or  outer  end 
of  the  same  gyrus  leads  to  movements  of  the  fore  limb,  and  we 
may  here  distinguish  between  an  area  stimulation  of  which 


FIG.  124.     THE  AREAS  OF  THE  CEREBRAL  CONVOLUTIONS  OF  THE  DOG,  ACCORDING 

TO   HlTZIG  AND   FRITSCH. 

(1)  A  The  area  for  the  muscles  of  the  neck.  (2)  -H  The  area  for  the  extension 
and  adduction  of  the  fore  limb.  (3)  +  The  area  for  the  flexion  and  rotation  of  the 
fore  limb.  (4)  ££  The  area  for  the  hind  limb.  Kunning  transversely  towards  and 
separating  (1)  and  (2)  from  (3)  and  (4)  is  seen  the  crucial  sulcus.  (5)  Q  The  facial 
area. 

(Fig.  124  +)  leads  to  flexion  of  the  fore  limb,  and  an  area 
(Fig.  124  -f)  stimulation  of  which  leads  to  extension  of  the  same 
limb.  In  a  similar  way  stimulation  of  other  areas  within  the 
'  motor '  region  leads  to  movements  of  this  kind  or  of  that  kind  of 
the  tail,  of  the  eyes,  of  the  mouth,  of  other  parts  of  the  face,  of 
the  tongue,  and  so  on.  Obviously  in  the  dog  this  region  of  the 
cortex  has  connections  with  the  skeletal  muscles  which  do  not 
obtain  between  other  regions  of  the  cortex  and  those  muscles; 
and  further,  the  region  in  question  is  topographically  differentiated, 
so  that  certain  areas  or  districts  of  the  region  are  specially  con- 
nected with  certain  skeletal  muscles  or  groups  of  muscles.  We 
may  speak  of  a  '  localisation  of  function '  in  this  region  as  compared 
with  other  regions  of  the  cortex,  and  in  the  several  areas  within 
the  region  as  compared  with  each  other. 

The  muscles  which  are  thus  thrown  into  contraction  are  the 
muscles  of  the  opposite  side  of  the  body.  When  '  the  four  limb 
area/  as  we  may  call  it,  of  the  right  hemisphere  is  stimulated,  it 
is  the  left  fore  limb  which  is  moved ;  and  so  with  the  other  areas ; 


CHAP.  IL]  THE   BRAIN.  1037 

it  is  only  in  exceptional  cases,  as  in  certain  movements  of  the  eyes, 
that  the  effect  is  bilateral ;  a  movement  confined  to  the  same  side 
as  that  stimulated  is  never  witnessed. 

The  results  are  most  clear  when  the  current  employed  as  a 
stimulus  is  not  stronger  than  is  just  sufficient  to  produce  the 
appropriate  movement  (roughly  speaking  a  current  just  perceptible 
to  the  tongue  of  the  operator  is  in  ordinary  cases  a  useful  one), 
and  when  the  cortex  is  in  good  nutritive  condition.  In  any  ex- 
periment the  results  obtained  by  the  earlier  stimulations,  soon 
after  the  cortex  has  been  exposed,  are  the  best ;  after  repeated 
stimulations  the  surface  is  apt  to  become  hyperaemic,  and  it  is 
then  frequently  observed  that  the  movements  resulting  from  the 
stimulation  of  a  particular  area  are  not  confined  to  the  appropriate 
muscles,  but  spread  to  the  corresponding  muscles  of  the  opposite 
side,  then  to  muscles  connected  with  other  cortical  areas,  and  at 
last  to  the  muscles  of  the  body  generally ;  at  the  same  time  the 
movements  lose  their  distinctive  purposeful  character  and  the 
animal  is  thrown  into  convulsions  of  an  epileptiform  kind.  It  not 
unfrequently  happens  that  an  experiment  has  to  be  stopped  in 
consequence  of  the  onset  of  these  epileptiform  convulsions.  The 
response  of  movement  to  stimulation  may  be  observed  while  the 
animal  is  under  the  moderate  influence  of  an  anaesthetic,  but  a 
too  profound  anaesthesia  lessens  or  annuls  the  effects. 

In  order  to  carry  out  a  closer  analysis  of  the  phenomena  it  is 
desirable  to  watch  or  record  the  contraction  of  a  particular  group 
of  muscles,  or  perhaps  better  still  a  particular  muscle,  e.  gr.  the 
area  for  extension  of  the  hind  limb  may  be  studied  by  help  of  the 
extensor  digitorum  communis  of  the  limb.  When  this  is  done 
the  following  important  facts  may  be  observed.  The  area  of 
cortex  having  been  found  which  gives  the  best  movements,  and 
the  stimulus  being  no  stronger  than  is  necessary,  isolation  of  the 
area  from  its  lateral  surroundings  by  a  circular  incision  carried  to 
some  little  depth  will  not  prevent  the  development  of  contractions 
in  the  muscle ;  but  these  do  cease,  even  without  the  circular 
incision,  if  by  a  horizontal  section  the  grey  cortex  is  separated 
from  the  subjacent  white  matter.  After  removal  of  the  cortex, 
stimulation  of  the  white  matter  underlying  the  area  produces  the 
appropriate  contraction ;  not  only  however  is  a  stronger  stimulus 
necessary,  but  also  the  latent  period,  that  is  the  time  intervening 
between  the  beginning  of  the  application  of  the  stimulating 
current  and  the  beginning  of  the  muscular  contraction  is  appre- 
ciably shortened.  The  appropriate  contractions  not  only  appear 
when  the  white  matter  immediately  below  the  cortex  is  stimulated, 
but  by  making  successive  horizontal  sections  and  stimulating  each 
in  turn,  the  effect  may,  so  to  speak,  be  traced  through  the  central 
white  matter  of  the  hemisphere  down  to  the  internal  capsule. 
We  may  conclude  from  these  results,  that  when  the  current  is 
applied  to  the  surface  of  the  cortex,  certain  parts  of  certain  struc- 


1038  CORTICAL   MOTOR   REGION.  [BOOK  in. 

tures  in  the  grey  matter  are  stimulated,  the  process  having  a 
marked  latent  period,  arid  that  as  the  outcome  of  the  changes 
induced  in  the  grey  matter,  impulses  pass  along  the  fibres  leading 
down  from  the  grey  matter  to  the  internal  capsule  and  so  by  the 
pedal  system  of  fibres  to  the  spinal  cord  and  motor  spinal  roots. 
The  anatomical  considerations  advanced  in  a  previous  section  lead 
us  to  suppose  that  the  fibres  in  question  belong  to  the  great  pyra- 
midal tract,  on  which  we  have  so  much  insisted ;  and  as  we  shall 
see  all  our  knowledge  confirms  this  view. 

It  must  not,  however,  be  supposed  that  the  several  areas 
stimulation  of  which  produces  each  its  distinctive  movement,  are 
in  the  dog  sharply  defined  from  each  other;  when  the  term  area 
for  extension  of  the  hind  limb  is  used  it  must  not  be  supposed 
that  the  area  can  be  defined  by  an  outline,  within  which  stimula- 
tion produces  nothing  but  extension  of  the  hind  limb,  and  outside 
which  stimulation  never  produces  extension  of  the  hind  limb.  All 
that  is  meant  is  that  extension  of  the  hind  limb  is  the  salient  and 
striking  result  of  stimulating  the  area.  When  we  study  the  various 
movements,  and  especially  perhaps  when  we  study,  by  help  of  a 
graphic  record,  the  contractions  of  various  individual  muscles 
resulting  from  the  stimulation  of  various  parts  of  the  motor  region, 
we  find  not  only  that  the  areas  for  particular  movements  or  parti- 
cular muscles  are  very  diffuse,  but  that  the  several  areas  largely 
overlap  each  other.  If  for  instance  we  were  to  map  out  on  the 
same  diagram  the  several  areas  belonging  to  four  or  five  muscles 
of  different  parts  of  the  body,  such  as  the  extensors  of  the  digits 
of  the  fore  and  of  the  hind  limb,  the  flexors  of  the  same,  and  the 
orbicular  muscle  of  the  eyelid,  that  is  to  say,  the  several  areas 
within  which  in  turn  stimulation  of  the  cortex  produced  contrac- 
tion of  the  particular  muscle,  the  overlapping  would  be  so  great  that 
the  whole  figure  would  appear  highly  confused.  In  a  similar  way 
the  excitable  motor  region  as  a  whole  would  gradually  merge  into, 
be  broken  up  into,  the  unexcitable  frontal,  occipital  and  temporal 
regions,  in  front,  behind  and  below.  In  other  words,  the  localisa- 
tion in  the  cortex  of  the  dog  is  to  a  marked  degree  imperfect. 

In  this  respect  the  dog,  corresponding  to  its  position  in  the 
animal  hierarchy,  is  intermediate  between  such  animals  as  the 
rabbit,  the  bird,  and  the  frog,  on  the  one  hand,  and  the  more 
highly  developed  monkey  on  the  other;  and  that  is  one  reason 
why  we  have  taken  the  dog  first  and  dwelt  so  long  upon  it.  In 
the  rabbit,  a  similar  localisation  may  be  observed,  but  far  less 
definite,  far  more  diffuse ;  it  becomes  still  less  in  the  bird,  and  is 
hardly  recognisable  in  the  frog.  It  will  not  be  profitable  to  dwell 
on  the  details  of  these  lower  animals ;  but  the  phenomena  of  the 
monkey,  leading  up  as  they  do  to  those  of  man,  call  for  special 
notice. 

§  655.  When  in  a  monkey,  in  an  individual  for  instance 
belonging  to  the  genus  Macacus,  the  surface  of  the  cerebrum  is 


CHAP.  IL]  THE   BRAIN.  1039 

explored  with  reference  to  the  effects  of  electric  stimulation,  it  is 
found  that  when  the  current  is  applied  to  the  precentral  or 
ascending  frontal  and  the  post-central  or  ascending  parietal 
convolutions  which  lie  respectively  in  front  of  and  behind  the 
important  central  fissure  or  fissure  of  Rolando  (cf.  Fig.  125), 
movements  of  the  fore  limb  follow.  The  'motor  area  for  the 
fore  limb '  thus  discovered  is  more  circumscribed  and  definite  than 
is  the  corresponding  area  in  the  dog.  Its  outline  (Fig.  126)  is 
roughly  that  of  a  truncated  triangle  bisected  by  the  central 
fissure,  with  the  broad  base  at  some  distance  from  the  mesial 
line,  and  the  truncated  apex  reaching  on  the  lateral  surface  of  the 
hemisphere  to  a  well-marked  bend  in  the  lower  part  of  the  central 
fissure.  Behind,  it  reaches  as  far  as  the  intra-parietal  fissure  which 
somewhat  sharply  defines  its  hind  border,  and  in  front  it  ceases  no 
less  definitely  at  some  little  distance  behind  the  precentral  fissure. 
Further  examination  shews  that  the  whole  area  is  divided  into 
areas  corresponding  to  movements  of  particular  parts  of  the  fore 
arm,  and  that  these  are  arranged  in  a  definite  relation  to  each 
other.  In  the  more  dorsal  part  of  the  area,  at  the  base  of  the 
triangle,  stimulation  produces  movements  of  the  shoulder 
(Fig.  126) ;  if  the  electrodes  be  shifted  ventrally  movements 
of  the  elbow  make  their  appearance  ;  if  still  more  ventrally, 
movements  of  the  wrist  come  in,  and  these  are  in  turn  succeeded 
ventrally  by  movements  of  the  digits  generally,  of  the  forefinger, 
and  lastly  of  the  thumb.  A  very  striking  experiment  may  be 
made  by  applying  a  current  of  suitable  strength,  first  at  the  lower, 
ventral  border  of  the  area,  and  then  gradually  advancing  upwards 
towards  the  mesial  line ;  the  thumb  is  moved  first,  then  the  fore- 
finger, then  the  rest  of  the  digits,  then  the  wrist,  next  the  elbow, 
and  lastly  the  shoulder.  Further,  in  certain  parts  of  the  area  the 
resulting  movement  is  flexion  of  the  appropriate  segment  of  the 
limb,  in  other  parts  extension,  in  certain  parts  abduction,  in  other 
parts  adduction,  and  so  on. 

Similar  exploration  shews  that  the  "  area  for  the  hind  limb," 
lies  on  the  median  side  of  the  area  for  the  fore  limb,  stretching 
besides  on  to  the  mesial  surface  along  the  marginal  convolution 
which  forms  the  dorsal  portion  of  the  wall  of  the  great  longitudinal 
fissure ;  it  reaches  as  far  back  as  the  intra-parietal  sulcus,  and  is 
succeeded  in  front  by  the  "  area  for  the  trunk  "  (Fig.  127).  Within 
this  general  area  for  the  hind  limb  we  may  similarly  distinguish 
special  areas  for  the  hip  (Figs.  126,  127)  in  the  front  portion,  for 
the  knee  and  ankle  behind  this,  and  for  the  digits  still  farther 
backwards,  the  area  for  the  great  toe  being  however  in  front  of 
the  area  for  the  other  digits. 

In  front  of  the  areas  for  the  limbs  and  trunk,  on  the  median 
dorsal  surface,  dipping  down  into  the  mesial  surface  along  the 
marginal  convolution  (Fig.  127)  and  reaching  laterally  on  the 
dorsal  lateral  surface  to  the  dorsal  extremity  of  the  precentral 

F.  66 


1040 


CORTICAL   MOTOR   REGION. 


[BOOK  in. 


sulcus  (Fig.  126),  is  the  "area  for  the  head,"  that  is  to  say  for 
movements  of  the  head  brought  about  by  contractions  of  the 
muscles  of  the  neck. 

Ventral  to  this  again,  in  front  of  the  precentral  sulcus  is  the 
"  area  for  the  eyes,"  that  is  to  say,  for  contractions  of  the  ocular 
muscles ;  and  behind  the  precentral  sulcus,  ventral  to  the  arm  area, 
lies  a  small  area  for  movements  of  the  eyelids,  brought  about  by 


FIG.  125.  OUTLINE  OF  BRAIN  OP  MONKEY  (MACACUS)  TO  SHEW  PRINCIPAL  SULCI 
(FISSURES)  AND  GYRI  (CONVOLUTIONS).  (Natural  size.)  (Sherrington  after 
Horsley  and  Schafer.) 

The  brain  figured  is  the  same  as  that  in  Fig.  126,  and  the  two  figures  should  be 
consulted  together.  Over  each  sulcus,  purposely  printed  very  thick,  the  name  is 
written  in  small  capitals,  over  each  gyrus  in  italics,  x  indicates  the  small  depres- 
sion, hardly  to  be  called  a  sulcus,  which  is  supposed  to  be  homologous  with 
the  superior  frontal  sulcus  of  man;  and  w,  y,  z  similarly  indicate  sulci  whose 
homologies  are  not  certain.  For  some  synonyms  see  Figs.  129,  130. 

contractions  of  the  orbicularis  muscle.  Ventral  to  this  again  is 
the  'area  for  the  face,'  in  which  we  may  distinguish  an  area  for 
the  mouth,  that  is  an  area  stimulation  of  which  produces  changes 
in  the  buccal  orifice,  opening,  shutting,  drawing  to  one  side  &c., 
and  an  area  for  movements  of  the  tongue.  These  two  areas 


CHAP,  ii.] 


THE   BRAIN. 


1041 


reach  downwards  to  the  fissure  of  Sylvius,  and  backwards  to  the 
line  of  the  intra-parietal  sulcus.  In  front  of  them,  occupying 
all  the  ventral  part  of  the  precentral  convolution  and  reaching 
forwards  as  far  as  the  precentral  sulcus,  where  it  meets  the  area 


TRUNK--... 


FIG.  126.  LEFT  HEMISPHERE  OP  THE  CEREBRUM  OF  MACACUS  MONKEY  VIEWED  FROM 
ITS  LEFT  SIDE,  AND  FROM  ABOVE.  Natural  size.  (Sherrington  after  Horsley  and 
Beevor.) 

The  figure  shews  the  positions  of  the  portions  of  the  cortex  concerned  with  move- 
ment of  various  parts,  and  with  the  senses  of  sight,  smell,  and  hearing.  The 
cortical  area  connected  with  the  movements  of  the  leg  is  shaded  vertically 
across,  that  with  the  movements  of  the  arm  horizontally,  and  that  with  the 
movements  of  the  trunk  in  a  slanting  direction;  the  area  connected  with 
movements  of  the  head  (neck),  face,  and  eyes  is  dotted.  The  course  of  the 
chief  fissures  is  indicated  by  single  lines. 

for  the  eyes,  lies  an  area  stimulation  of  which  produces  movements 
of  the  pharynx  or  larynx  as  well  as  the  mouth  or  face,  and  which 
may  be  divided  into  areas  for  mastication,  for  swallowing,  and  for 
the  production  of  the  voice. 

We  might  speak  of  these  several  areas  in  another  way  by 

66—2 


1042 


CORTICAL   MOTOR   REGION. 


[BOOK  in. 


referring  to  the  nerves  concerned  in  carrying  out  the  several 
movements,  though  in  doing  so  we  must  remember  that  there  is 
not  an  exact  correspondence  between  the  relative  position  of  a 
muscle  along  the  axis  of  the  body  or  along  the  axis  of  a  limb  and 
the  relative  position  along  the  cerebrospinal  axis  of  the  nerve  or 
nerves  governing  the  muscle.  We  may  however,  adopting  this 
method,  note  that  the  sacral  and  lumbar  nerves  are  represented  by 


FIG.  127.  MESIAL  ASPECT  OF  THE  LEFT  HALF  OF  THE  BRAIN  OF  MACACUS,  DISPLAYED 
BY  SECTION  IN  THE  MEDIAN  SAGITTAL  PLANE  AND  REMOVAL  OF  THE  CEREBELLUM. 
Natural  size.  (Sherrington  after  Horsley  and  Beevor.) 

The  hatched  and  stippled  parts  of  the  surface  shew  the  regions  of  the  cortex 
connected  with  movements  of  the  foot,  knee,  hip,  tail,  trunk,  and  neck 
respectively.  The  several  positions  of  the  areas  of  cortex  connected  with 
vision  and  smell  and  with  cutaneous  sensation  are  indicated  by  the  appropriate 
words. 

The  plane  of  section  has  passed  through  the  corpus  callosum,  cc,  cc,  cc,  and  through 
the  anterior  commissure,  c,  sparing  the  left  pillar  of  the  fornix,  F\  behind  it 
has  bisected  the  anterior  part  of  the  pons,  laying  open  the  aqueduct,  Aq.  (iter 
a  tertio  ad  quartum  ventriculum).  Pons,  the  left  half  of  the  pons  in  frontal 
section.  Op.  the  optic  commissure  cut  across. 

III.  the  root  of  the  third  cranial  nerve. 

FR.  the  frontal  pole,*  OC.  the  occipital  pole;  Cn.  the  cuneus,  Pen.  the  precuneus; 
G.  fn.  G.  fn.  G.  fn.  the  gyrus  fornicatus ;  the  unlettered  fissure  seen  to  form 
the  upper  boundary  of  this  gyrus  in  its  supra-callosal  part  is  the  calloso- 
marginal,  Po.  f.  the  parieto-occipital  fissure. 

the  most  mesial  portion  of  the  whole  motor  area  and  by  the  hind 
division  of  this  mesial  portion ;  that  the  lumbar  and  thoracic  nerves 
are  represented  by  the  front  division  of  the  same  mesial  portion ; 
that  the  upper  thoracic  with  the  lower  cervical  nerves  belong 
to  a  region  lying  lateral  to,  and  the  upper  cervical  nerves  to  one 
lying  in  front  of  the  preceding  area ;  and  lastly  that  the  remaining 
lateral  and  ventral  portions  of  the  whole  motor  region  appertain  to 
the  cranial  nerves.  But  the  topographical  differentiation  does 


CHAP,  ii.]  THE   BRAIN.  1043 

not  come  out  so  clearly  by  this  method,  as  by  that  of  taking  for 
our  guide  distinctive  movements  of  the  several  parts  of  the  body. 

It  will  be  observed  that  all  these  areas  taken  together,  repre- 
sented by  the  portion  of  Figs.  126,  127  shaded  in  one  way  or 
another,  occupy  chiefly  the  parietal  region  of  the  cerebral  surface 
though  they  also  reach  into  the  frontal  region.  Stimulation  of  the 
frontal  region  in  front  of  this  motor  area  or  of  the  occipital  region 
behind,  whether  on  the  lateral  or  on  the  mesial  surface,  or  of 
the  temporal  region,  whether  also  on  the  lateral  or  on  the  mesial 
surface,  or  of  the  gyms  fornicatus  (Fig.  127)  connecting  the  frontal 
and  occipital  regions  on  the  mesial  surface,  and  running  ventral 
to  the  marginal  gyrus,  does  not  give  rise  to  movements ;  or  to  be 
more  exact,  does  not  give  rise  to  movements  comparable  to  those 
just  described  as  resulting  from  stimulation  of  various  parts  of  the 
motor  region.  Movements  do  take  place  when  certain  parts  of  the 
occipital  or  of  the  temporal  region  are  stimulated,  but  these  are 
not  only  feeble  and  experimentally  uncertain,  but  appear  to  be  of 
a  different  nature  from  those  resulting  from  stimulation  of  the 
motor  region ;  it  will  be  convenient  to  speak  of  the  nature  and 
meaning  of  this  kind  of  movement  when  we  come  to  discuss  the 
development  of  sensations. 

§  656.  It  is  obvious  from  the  foregoing  that  the  mechanisms 
for  the  development  of  these  movements  of  cerebral  origin  are  far 
more  highly  differentiated  in  the  monkey  than  in  the  dog.  But 
even  in  the  monkey  (Macacus  and  allied  forms)  the  differentiation 
is  still  very  incomplete.  If  we  explore  for  instance  the  area  for  the 
wrist  we  find  that  its  limits  are  ill-defined.  In  some  parts  of  the 
area  we  obtain  movements  of  the  wrist  only,  but  in  other  parts  of 
the  area  stimulation  produces  not  only  movements  of  the  wrist, 
but  also  of  the  shoulder  or  of  the  digits,  or  of  the  neck ;  and  so 
with  the  other  areas. 

If,  however,  not  a  Macacus  or  other  ordinary  monkey,  but  the 
more  highly  developed  ourang  otang  be  taken  as  the  subject  of 
experiments,  the  differentiation  is  found  to  be  distinctly  advanced ; 
the  several  areas  are  more  sharply  defined,  and  what  is  important 
to  note,  the  respective  areas  tend  to  be  separated  from  each  by 
portions  of  cortex,  stimulation  of  which  gives  rise  to  no  movement 
at  all. 

The  opportunities  of  stimulating  the  cortex  of  man  himself  have 
been  few  and  far  between,  and  have  for  the  most  part  been  con- 
ducted under  unfavourable  circumstances ;  but  as  far  as  the  results 
so  obtained  go,  they  shew  that  the  topographical  distribution  of 
areas  for  the  several  movements  is  carried  out  on  the  same  plan  as 
in  the  monkey  (we  are  purposely  confining  ourselves  now  to  the 
results  of  artificial  stimulation) ;  and  moreover,  justify  the  con- 
clusion, which  a  priori  reasons  would  lead  us  to  adopt,  that  in  man 
the  differentiation  is  advanced  still  farther  than  in  the  monkey. 

Thus  when  we  survey  a  series  of  brains  in  succession,  from  the 


1044  MOVEMENTS   OF  CORTICAL   ORIGIN.     [BOOK  m. 

more  lowly  frog,  through  the  bird,  the  rabbit,  the  dog,  and  other 
lower  mammals  up  to  the  monkey,  the  anthropoid  ape,  and  so  to 
man  himself,  we  find  an  increasing  differentiation  of  the  cerebral 
cortex,  by  which  certain  areas  of  the  cortex  are  brought  into 
special  connection  with  certain  skeletal  or  other  muscles  in  such 
a  way  that  stimulation  of  a  particular  portion  of  the  grey  matter 
gives  rise  to  a  particular  movement  and  to  that  alone. 

§  657.  In  treating  of  the  structure  of  the  brain  we  spoke 
(§  632)  of  the  pyramidal  tract  as  starting  from  the  motor  region 
of  the  cortex ;  and  it  is  obvious  that  the  fibres  of  this  tract  must 
be  concerned  in  the  development  of  the  movements  which  we 
have  just  described.  When  the  movements  are  brought  about 
by  stimulation  of  the  fibres  in  some  part  of  their  course,  in  the 
internal  capsule  for  instance,  there  can  be  no  doubt  that  the 
stimulation  starts  impulses  which,  travelling  down  the  tract  to 
the  origins  of  certain  cranial  or  spinal  nerves,  in  some  way  give 
rise  to  coordinate  motor  impulses  along  the  motor  fibres  of  the 
nerves;  and  we  may  with  reason  speak  of  the  impulses  then 
passing  along  the  tract  as  motor  or  efferent  in  nature.  When  the 
stimulus  is  applied  direct  to  the  cortex,  we  may  assume  that 
processes,  started  in  the  grey  matter,  eventuate  in  similar  efferent 
impulses  along  the  fibres  of  the  tract.  All  the  evidence  leads  us 
to  regard  this  tract  as  an  efferent  tract. 

When  the  spinal  cord  is  divided  in  the  lower  dorsal  region  and 
the  electrodes  of  an  electrometer  are  brought  into  connection  with 
the  transverse  cut  surface  and  with  some  point  of  the  longitudinal 
surface  above,  the  electrometer  gives  evidence  of  currents  of 
action  (manifested  as  negative  variations  of  a  demarcation  current 
or  current  of  rest,  §  67)  whenever  the  motor  area  of  the  hind 
limb  is  stimulated,  but  not  when  other  parts  of  the  cortex  are 
stimulated.  We  have  already  said  that  stimulation  of  any  part  of 
the  motor  region  may  under  abnormal  conditions  give  rise  to 
general  epileptiform  convulsions ;  when  these  occur  during  such 
an  experiment  as  the  above,  currents  of  action  manifest  themselves 
in  the  lower  dorsal  cord,  whether  the  stimulation  giving  rise  to 
the  convulsions  be  applied  to  the  area  for  the  hind  limb  or  to  any 
part  of  the  motor  region.  It  has  been  further  observed  that  the 
currents  of  action  developed  within  the  spinal  cord  tally  in  a  very 
exact  manner  with  the  muscular  movements.  The  convulsions 
begin  with  a  sustained  '  tonic  '  contraction  of  the  muscles,  and  the 
electrometer  shews  a  similar  sustained  current  of  action ;  this  is 
followed  by  rhythmic  movements  of  the  muscles,  accompanied  by 
corresponding  rhythmic  movements  of  the  mercury  of  the  electro- 
meter. Without  insisting  too  much  on  the  exact  interpretation 
of  these  results  we  may  take  them  as  at  least  shewing  that,  when 
the  motor  region  of  the  cortex  is  excited,  nervous  impulses  accom- 
panied by  "  currents  of  action  "  pass  downward  along  the  fibres  of 
the  pyramidal  tract. 


CHAP.  IL]  THE   BRAIN.  1045 

The  results  of  stimulating  the  fibres  of  the  tract  in  their  course 
through  the  corona  radiata  and  the  internal  capsule,  and  the 
results  obtained  by  studying  the  degenerations  following  upon 
injury  to  or  removal  of  the  several  parts  of  the  cortical  motor  region, 
agree  in  marking  out  the  paths  taken  by  the  several  constituents 
of  the  tract  through  the  central  white  matter  of  the  hemisphere, 
the  corona  radiata  and  the  capsule.  Comparing  Figs.  126,  127 
with  Figs.  121,  122  and  123  it  will  be  seen  that  the  portion  of  the 
tract  destined  for  the  cranial  nerves,  and  so  for  the  movements  of 
the  eyes,  the  mouth,  face,  tongue,  pharynx  and  larynx,  starting 
from  the  ventral  parts  of  the  more  frontal  district  of  the  motor 
region,  take  up  their  position  at  the  knee  of  the  internal  capsule ; 
and  the  portion  destined  for  those  upper  cervical  nerves  which 
carry  out  movements  of  the  head  through  the  muscles  of  the 
neck,  starting  from  the  extreme  frontal  and  dorsal  parts  of  the 
area,  is  also  apparently  directed  to  the  knee  of  the  capsule.  The 
rest  of  the  tract,  starting  from  the  part  of  the  area  lying  at  once 
behind  and  mesial  to  the  above,  occupies  in  the  capsule  a  position 
posterior  to  them  in  the  hind  limb  of  the  capsule  ;  and  it  will  be 
observed  that  the  tract  for  the  fore  limb  which  begins  on  the 
surface  lateral  of  the  tracts  for  the  trunk  and  hind  limb,  shifts  its 
course  in  relation  to  theirs,  so  that  in  the  capsule  it  is  in  front  of 
them,  not  lateral  to  them.  It  may  further  be  observed  that  while 
in  the  tracts  for  the  trunk  and  hind  limb  the  same  fore  and  aft 
order  which  obtains  on  the  surface  is  reproduced  in  the  capsule, 
even  apparently  to  the  strange  precedence  of  the  ankle  over  *  the 
knee,  the  order  of  the  several  elements  in  the  fore  limb  tract 
which  is  lateral  on  the  surface  becomes  regularly  fore  and  aft  in 
the  capsule.  In  the  capsule  the  several  elements  are  arranged  in 
a  linear  order,  corresponding  broadly  to  that  of  the  distribution  of 
the  muscles  along  the  longitudinal  axis  of  the  body ;  on  the  cortex 
they  are  disposed  in  an  order  the  cause  of  which  is  at  present  not 
very  clear,  but  which  is  probably  determined  by  the  respective 
relations  of  the  several  parts  of  the  motor  region  to  the  functional 
activity  of  the  other  parts  of  the  cortex.  In  the  shifting  from  the 
one  order  to  the  other,  the  several  constituent  fibres,  as  we  have 
said,  describe  a  somewhat  peculiar  course ;  and  when  we  remember, 
as  stated  in  §  632,  that  the  order  shewn  in  Fig.  121  is  only  the 
order  obtaining  at  one  particular  level  of  the  capsule,  and  that 
from  the  dorsal  beginnings  of  the  capsule  in  the  corona  radiata  to 
its  ventral  end  in  the  pes,  the  capsule  is  continually  changing  in 
form,  and  its  fibres  therefore  continually  shifting  their  relations  to 
each  other,  the  whole  course  of  the  several  fibres  of  the  tract  from 
their  origin  in  the  cortex  until  they  are  gathered  up  into  the 
central  portion  of  the  pes  (Fig.  114  Py)  must  be  a  very  compli- 
cated one. 

When  the  area  of  one  hemisphere  is  stimulated,  the  movement 
which  results  is  in  most  cases  seen  on  the  other  side  of  the  body, 


1046  MOVEMENTS   OF   CORTICAL   ORIGIN.     [BOOK  m. 

and  on  that  other  side  alone.  Thus  when  the  area  for  the  fore 
limb  is  stimulated  on  the  left  hemisphere  it  is  the  right  fore  limb 
which  is  moved.  This  is  in  accordance  with  what  we  have  learnt 
of  the  pyramidal  tract  and  its  ultimate  entire  decussation  before  it 
reaches  the  motor  nerves,  the  decussation  either  occurring  mas- 
sively as  in  the  case  of  the  crossed  pyramidal  tract,  or  in  a  more 
scattered  manner  along  the  upper  part  of  the  spinal  cord  in  the 
case  of  the  direct  pyramidal  tract ;  and,  as  we  have  seen,  there  is 
a  similar  decussation  for  such  part  of  the  pyramidal  tract  as  is 
connected  with  the  cranial  nerves  above  the  decussation  of  the 
pyramids.  Except  in  the  case  of  certain  areas  for  movements 
naturally  bilateral  of  which  we  shall  speak  presently,  the  move- 
ment is  normally  on  the  crossed  side,  and  on  the  crossed  side  only. 
Under  abnormal  conditions  however  the  limb  of  the  other  side, 
that  is  of  the  same  side  as  the  hemisphere  stimulated,  may  move 
also.  But  such  an  abnormal  movement  of  the  same  side  has  not 
the  same  characters  as  the  proper  movement  of  the  crossed  limb. 
Instead  of  being  an  orderly  coordinate  movement,  it  is  a  more 
simple,  either  tetanic  or  perhaps  tonic,  or  rhythmic,  clonic,  con- 
traction of  the  muscles.  Obviously  its  mechanism  is  of  a  different 
nature  from  that  by  which  the  proper  movement  of  the  crossed 
limb  is  effected ;  but  it  is  important  to  bear  in  mind  that  a  move- 
ment of  the  uncrossed  limb  may  take  place  ;  and  further  that,  the 
abnormal  conditions  continuing,  similar  movements  of  an  uncoor- 
dinated character  may  spread  to  the  hind  limb  and  other  parts  of 
the  crossed  side,  though  the  stimulation  be  still  confined  to  the 
arm  area,  then  to  other  parts  of  the  uncrossed  side,  until  as  we 
have  said  the  whole  body  is  thrown  into  epileptiform  convulsions. 
This  feature  must  not  be  forgotten.  In  fact  it  may  be  fairly 
insisted  upon  that  while  we  may  speak  of  a  particular  coordinate 
movement  as  being  the  normal  outcome  of  an  ordinary  careful 
stimulation  of  a  particular  area  in  a  normal  condition,  it  is  no  less 
true  that  diffuse  uncoordinated  movements,  culminating  in  general 
epileptiform  convulsions,  are  the  natural  outcome  of  the  stimula- 
tion of  any  area  in  an  abnormal  condition.  And  in  attempting  to 
form  any  opinion  of  the  nature  of  the  first  act,  we  must  bear  the 
second  in  mind. 

As  we  said  above,  the  movements  resulting  from  cortical 
stimulation  are  most  conveniently  described  in  terms  of  parts  of 
the  body,  of  the  arm,  of  the  thumb,  of  the  tongue,  &c.  The 
movements  of  the  same  part  may  be  further  distinguished  by 
means  of  the  nomenclature  usually  adopted  in  speaking  of  mus- 
cular movements,  such  as  flexion,  extension,  abduction,  adduction, 
&c. ;  so  that,  within  the  area  bearing  the  name  of  some  particular 
part,  such  as  the  wrist  for  instance,  we  have  to  distinguish  an  area 
for  the  flexion,  and  another  for  the  extension  of  that  joint ;  and  in 
like  manner  in  reference  to  other  parts.  But  it  will  be  readily 
understood  that  it  is  easier  to  map  out  the  area  for  a  particular 


CHAP.  IL]  THE   BRAIN.  1047 

part  than  to  distinguish  the  areas  corresponding  to  the  several 
movements  of  that  part.  Hence  the  nomenclature  usually  adopted 
in  speaking  of  the  motor  region  is  one  based  on  the  parts  of  the 
body  moved  rather  than  on  the  character  of  the  movements.  The 
more  closely  however  the  movements  in  question  are  studied,  the 
more  probable  it  appears  that  the  localisation  which  obtains  in 
the  cortex  is  essentially  a  localisation  corresponding  not  to  parts 
of  the  body,  or  to  nerves,  or  to  muscles,  but  to  movements.  In 
considering  this  point  it  must  be  remembered  how  rude  and 
barbarous  a  method  of  stimulation  is  that  of  applying  electrodes 
to  the  surface  of  the  grey  matter  compared  with  the  natural 
stimulation  which  takes  place  during  cerebral  action ;  the  one 
probably  is  about  as  much  alike  the  other,  as  is  striking  the  keys 
of  a  piano  at  a~  distance  with  a  broomstick  to  the  execution  of  a 
skilled  musician.  Were  it  in  our  power  to  stimulate  the  cortex 
in  any  way  at  all  approaching  the  natural  method,  we  should  in 
all  probability  arrive  at  two  results ;  on  the  one  hand  we  should 
be  able  to  produce  at  will  a  variety  of  movements  of  different 
degrees  of  complexity,  some  very  simple,  others  very  complex,  and 
for  these  we  should  have  to  use  names  suggested  by  the  characters 
and  purpose  of  each  movement,  and  by  these  alone ;  on  the  other 
hand  we  should  find  very  decided  limits  to  the  number  and  kind  of 
movements  which  we  could  evoke,  limits  fixed  in  the  case  of  each 
subject  partly  by  inherited  organisation,  partly  by  the  training  of 
the  individual. 

Some  such  results  of  refined  experimentation  are  indeed  already 
foreshadowed  by  the  rude  results  of  our  present  rough  methods. 
The  movements  which  usually  follow  stimulation  of  the  motor 
region,  and  which  we  have  described  as  flexion,  &c.,  are,  so  to 
speak,  the  elementary  factors  of  ordinary  bodily  movements  the 
detached  and  imperfect  chords  of  a  musical  piece  ;  and  in  the  fol- 
lowing facts  relating  to  their  production  we  can  recognize  the 
influences  of  organisation  and  habit.  As  we  have  said,  stimulation 
of  the  motor  area  of  one  hemisphere  produces  movements,  as  a 
rule,  which  are  limited  to  one  side  of  the  body,  and  that  the 
opposite  side.  Now  both  in  ourselves  and  in  the  higher  animals 
a  large  number  of  bodily  movements,  especially  of  the  limbs,  are 
habitually  unilateral ;  and,  putting  aside  the  question  why  there 
should  be  two  halves  of  the  brain,  and  why  the  one  half  of  the 
brain  should  be  associated  with  the  cross  half  of  the  bodily,  we 
may  recognize  in  the  unilateral  crossed  movement  resulting  from 
stimulation  of  the  cortex  in  accordance  with  natural  habits.  But 
some  movements  of  the  body  are  ordinarily  bilateral ;  the  two  eyes, 
for  instance,  are  ordinarily  moved  together,  and  the  two  sides 
of  the  trunk  move  together  very  much  more  frequently  than  do 
the  two  fore  limbs  or  the  two  hind  limbs.  And  in  accordance 
with  this  we  find  that  stimulation  of  the  motor  area  for  the  eyes 
on  either  hemisphere  produces  movements  of  both  eyes,  and  stimu- 


1048  MOVEMENTS   OF   CORTICAL   ORIGIN.     [BOOK  in. 

lation  of  the  trunk  area  of  one  hemisphere  is  also  very  apt  to 
produce  bilateral  action  of  the  trunk  muscles ;  in  such  instances 
the  movements  on  both  sides  are  quite  normal  movements.  We 
may  incidentally  remark  that  removal  of  the  trunk  area  leads  to  a 
good  deal  of  bilateral  degeneration,  that  is,  to  degeneration  of 
strands  in  the  pyramidal  tracts  of  both  sides,  whereas  such  a 
bilateral  degeneration  is  comparatively  scanty  after  removal  of  the 
leg  or  arm  area. 

That  it  is  the  movement  and  not  the  part  moved  which  is,  so 
to  speak,  represented  on  the  cortex  is  further  shewn  by  the  relative 
magnitudes  of  the  several  cortical  areas  when  they  are  mapped 
out  according  to  parts  of  the  body.  The  area  for  the  arm,  for 
instance,  cf.  Figs.  126,  127,  is,  so  to  speak,  enormous  compared  to 
that  of  the  trunk  when  the  relative  bulks  of  these-  two  parts  of 
the  body  are  considered ;  and  within  the  arm  area  itself  the  space 
occupied  by  the  thumb  and  fore-finger  and  digits  is,  bulk  for  bulk, 
out  of  proportion  to  the  space  allotted  to  the  shoulder ;  so  also  the 
area  for  the  eyes  or  for  the  mouth  is  out  of  proportion  to  the  size 
of  those  organs.  But  these  relative  sizes  of  the  respective  areas 
become  intelligible  when  we  bear  in  mind  relative  mobility,  nim- 
bleness  and  delicacy  of  execution  ;  in  these  respects  the  shoulder  is 
far  behind  the  thumb,  while  the  eyes  and  mouth  surpass  most 
other  parts  of  the  body. 

We  are  brought  yet  a  step  further  when  we  compare,  in  respect 
of  the  cortical  motor  region,  animals  of  different  grades  of  organi- 
sation ;  and  the  results  thus  obtained  lead  us  to  the  conclusion 
that  the  motor  region  is  correlated  not  to  movements  in  general, 
but  to  movements  of  a  particular  kind.  Taking  in  series  the 
rabbit,  the  dog,  the  monkey  and  man,  we  find  in  passing  from 
one  to  the  other,  an  increase  in  prominence  and  in  differentiation 
of  the  motor  region  accompanied  by  an  increase  in  the  bulk  of 
the  pyramidal  tract ;  among  the  many  striking  differences  be- 
tween the  brains  of  these  several  animals,  these  two  features, 
the  increasing  complexity  of  the  motor  region,  and  the  increasing 
size  of  the  pyramidal  tract,  are  among  the  most  striking.  The 
size  of  the  pyramidal  tract  is  itself  correlated  to  the  complexity 
of  the  motor  region,  and,  being  the  more  easily  determined, 
may  be  used  as  indicating  both ;  the  difference  in  the  size 
of  the  pyramidal  tract  in  these  animals  is  seen  all  along  the 
whole  length  of  the  cord  (Fig.  128).  Now  as  regards  mere  quan- 
tity of  movement,  if  we  may  use  such  an  expression,  the  differences 
between  these  animals  are  of  no  great  moment.  If  we  were 
to  take  the  amount  of  energy  expended  as  movement  in  twenty- 
four  hours  per  gramme  of  muscle  present  in  the  body  in  each 
of  the  four  cases,  we  should  certainly  not  find  any  correspon- 
dence between  that  and  the  size  of  the  pyramidal  tract.  If 
however  we  take  a  particular  kind  of  movement,  what  we  may 
perhaps  call  skilled  movement,  that  is  movement  carried  out  by 


CHAP,  ii.] 


THE    BRAIN. 


1049 


means  of  intricate  changes  in  the  central  nervous  system,  we  do 
find  a  remarkable  parallelism  in  the  above  cases  between  the 
amount  of  such  skilled  movement  entering  into  the  daily  life  of 
the  individual  and  the  size  of  the  pyramidal  tract.  In  these  two 
respects  man  is  much  above  the  monkey,  and  the  monkey  far  above 


Py.d 


MAN 


MONKEY 


OOC 


FIG.  128. 


DIAGRAM    TO    ILLUSTRATE    THE    RELATIVE    SlZE    OF   THE    PYBAMIDAL    TRACT 

IN  THE  DOG,  MONKEY  AND  MAN.     (Sherrington.) 

The  figure  shews  in  outline  the  lateral  half  of  the  cord,  at  the  level  of  the  fifth 
thoracic  nerve,  in  A.  Man,  B.  Monkey,  C.  Dog;  A  is  a  reproduction  of  D5  in 
Fig.  104;  B  and  C  are  drawn  of  the  same  size  as  A.  Py.,  shaded  obliquely, 
the  pyramidal  tract;  the  depth  of  shading  indicates  that  the  tract  is  more 
crowded  with  true  pyramidal  fibres  as  well  as  larger  in  A  than  in  B,  and  in 
B  than  in  (7.  In  B,  Pif  is  an  outlying  portion  of  the  pyramidal  tract  separated 
from  the  rest  by  the  cerebellar  tract.  Py.d.  the  direct  pyramidal  tract,  present 
in  man  only.  The  grey  matter  seems  relatively  large  in  G  because  the  section 
was  taken  from  a  very  young  puppy. 

the  dog.  We  may  conclude  then  that  the  cortical  motor  region  is 
in  some  way  especially  concerned  with  the  kind  of  movement 
which  we  have  called  'skilled.' 

§  658.  These  skilled  movements  are  to  a  large  extent,  though 
not  exclusively,  voluntary  movements.  We  have  in  a  previous 
section  seen  reason  to  believe  that  the  cerebral  cortex  is  in  some 
way  especially  associated  with  the  development  of  voluntary 
movements.  Putting  together  this  conclusion  and  the  conclusions 
just  arrived  at  we  are  naturally  led  to  the  further  conclusion  that 
the  cortical  motor  region,  with  the  pyramidal  tract  belonging  to 
it,  plays  an  important  part  in  carrying  out  voluntary  movements. 
Do  other  facts  support  this  view,  and  if  so,  what  light  do  they 
throw  on  the  question  as  to  what  part  and  what  kind  of  part  the 
motor  region  thus  plays  ? 

In  this  connection  we  naturally  desire  to  know  what  are  the 
results  of  removing  from  an  otherwise  intact  animal  the  whole 
motor  region,  and  more  especially  this  or  that  particular  portion 
of  it.  Before  proceeding  further,  however,  we  may  once  more  call 
attention  to  the  caution  given  in  §582,  and  repeated  in  §640; 
indeed  when  we  consider  the  high  organisation  and  complex 
functions  which  obviously  belong  to  the  cortex,  when  we  bear  in 


1050  REMOVAL   OF   CORTICAL   AREAS.        [BOOK  in. 

mind  that  it  appears  to  govern,  and  must  therefore  be  bound  by 
close  ties  to  almost  all  the  rest  of  the  central  nervous  system,  we 
must  be  prepared  to  find  after  removing  a  portion  of  cortex  that 
the  pure  'deficiency'  phenomena,  those  which  result  from  the 
mere  absence  of  a  piece  of  the  cortex,  are  largely  obscured  by 
the  other  effects  of  the  operation. 

In  the  rabbit  the  results  have  been  almost  purely  negative. 
When  in  this  animal  the  part  of  the  cortex  which  may  be  con- 
sidered as  the  motor  region  is  removed,  nothing  remarkable  is 
observed  in  the  movements  of  the  animal.  We  can  hardly  suppose 
that  the  operations  of  the  central  nervous  system  are  the  same  in 
an  operated  as  in  an  intact  animal,  and  the  differences  induced 
ought  to  be  betrayed  by  the  movements  of  the  body;  but  at  present 
they  have  escaped  observation. 

In  the  dog  the  removal  of  an  area  is  followed  by  a  loss  or 
diminution  of  voluntary  movement  in  the  corresponding  part  of 
the  body.  When,  for  instance,  the  area  for  the  fore  limb  is 
removed  from  the  left  hemisphere,  the  right  fore  limb  is  com- 
pletely or  partially  'paralysed.'  In  carrying  out  its  ordinary 
movements  the  operated  animal  makes  little  or  no  use  of  its  right 
fore  limb.  But  this  state  of  things  is  temporary  only.  After  a 
while  the  animal  regains  power  over  the  limb,  and  in  successful 
cases  recovery  is  so  complete  that  it  is  impossible  to  point  out 
in  the  limb  any  appreciable  deviation  from  the  normal  use.  And 
careful  examination  after  death  has  shewn  not  only  that  the  area 
had  been  wholly  removed,  but  also  that  there  was  no  regeneration 
of  the  lost  parts;  the  removal  of  the  cortex  leads  in  such  cases,  as 
usual,  to  degeneration  of  the  corresponding  strand  in  the  pyra- 
midal tract  right  away  from  the  cerebral  surface  to  the  endings  of 
the  strand  in  the  cervical  and  dorsal  spinal  cord.  Nor  can  it  be 
urged  in  such  cases  that  diffused  remnants  of  the  arm  area  had 
been  left  in  the  remaining  parts  of  the  motor  region;  for  the 
whole  motor  region  has  been  removed,  and  yet  the  animal  has 
recovered  to  such  an  extent  that  a  casual  observer  could  detect  no 
differences  between  the  movements  of  the  two  sides  of  the  body. 
Closer  examination  did  disclose  certain  imperfections  of  move- 
ment ;  but  the  operation  had  involved  injury  to  or  produced 
changes  in  structures  other  than  the  motor  region,  and  the  imper- 
fections might  have  been  due  to  the  additional  damage.  Nor  can 
it  be  urged  that,  in  such  a  case,  where  one  side  is  removed,  the 
remaining  hemisphere  takes  on  double  functions;  for  the  greater 
part  of  the  motor  areas  have  been  removed  on  both  sides,  and  yet 
the  animal's  movements  have  been  so  far  apparently  complete 
that  a  casual  observer  would  see  nothing  strange  in  them.  Again, 
the  whole  motor  region  has  been  removed  from  one  hemisphere  in 
a  young  puppy,  and  some  time  later  when  the  movements  seemed 
to  have  recovered  their  normal  condition,  the  removal  of  the 
motor  region  of  the  other  hemisphere  has  produced  merely  a 


CHAP,  ii.]  THE   BRAIN.  1051 

paralysis  of  the  crossed  side  of  the  body,  and  that  as  before  only 
of  a  temporary  character. 

Two  things  have  to  be  noted  here.  In  the  first  place  the 
removal  of  an  area  does  affect  the  movements  which  are  brought 
about  by  stimulating  that  area,  it  leads  to  their  disappearance  or 
at  least  to  great  diminution  of  them  ;  and  this  affords  an  addi- 
tional argument  that  the  connection  between  the  area  and  the 
movement  is  a  real  and  important  one.  In  the  second  place,  the 
physiological  effect  is  temporary  only,  though  the  anatomical 
results  of  the  operation  are  permanent,  for  the  cortex  is  never 
renewed,  and  the  pyramidal  tract  degenerates  along  its  whole 
length,  never  to  be  restored ;  this  shews  that  we  have  to  deal  here 
with  events  of  a  very  complex  character.  When  a  particular 
movement  results  from  stimulation  of  the  appropriate  cortical 
area,  we  may  be  sure  that  whatever  takes  place  in  the  cortex  and 
along  the  pyramidal  tract,  motor  impulses,  duly  coordinated,  pass 
along  certain  anterior  roots  to  certain  muscles  ;  and  we  know  that 
if  we  removed  a  sufficient  length  of  each  of  those  anterior  roots 
that  particular  movement  would  be  lost  for  the  rest  of  the  life  of 
the  individual.  We  may  therefore  infer  that  the  events  which, 
whatever  be  their  exact  nature,  taking  place  in  the  cortex  and 
along  the  pyramidal  tract  lead  ultimately  to  the  issue  of  motor 
impulses  along  the  anterior  roots,  differ  essentially  from  the  events 
attending  the  transmission  of  ordinary  motor  impulses. 

In  the  case  of  the  monkey,  the  results  of  removing  parts  of  the 
cortical  motor  region  have  not  been  so  accordant  as  in  the  case  of 
the  dog.  The  two  animals  agree  perfectly  in  so  far  that  the 
removal  of  a  particular  area  leads,  as  an  immediate  result,  to  the 
loss  of  the  corresponding  movement ;  but  while  in  some  instances 
recovery  of  the  movement  has  in  the  monkey  as  in  the  dog  after  a 
while  taken  place,  in  other  instances  the  '  paralysis '  has  appeared  to 
be  permanent.  As  a  rule  the  paralysis  caused  by  a  large  lesion 
is  not  only  more  extensive,  but  also  of  longer  duration  than  that 
caused  by  a  small  one  ;  and  natural  bilateral  movements,  as  of  the 
eyes,  reappear  earlier  than  unilateral  movements.  The  facts 
however  within  our  knowledge  relating  to  the  permanence  of 
the  effect  are  neither  numerous  nor  exact  enough  to  justify  at 
present  a  definite  conclusion.  On  the  one  hand  the  positive 
cases  where  recovery  has  taken  place  are  of  more  value  than  the 
negative  ones,  since  in  the  latter  the  recovery  may  have  been 
hindered  by  concomitant  events  of  a  nature  which  we  may  call 
accidental ;  and  it  is  at  least  a  priori  most  unlikely  that  the 
pyramidal  tract  mechanism,  if  we  may  use  the  expression,  though 
it  may  differ  in  the  monkey  and  the  dog  in  degree  of  development, 
differs  so  essentially  in  kind  that  damage  of  it  leads  in  the  one 
case  to  permanent,  and  in  the  other  to  mere  temporary  loss  of 
function.  We  may  add  that  we  should  further  expect  to  meet 
in  the  monkey  with  more  prominent  and  more  lasting  com- 


1052  CORTICAL   MOTOR   REGION   IN   MAN.     [BOOK  in. 

plications  due  to  the  subsidiary  effects  of  the  operation,  and  it 
may  be  doubted  whether  in  any  of  the  recorded  experiments  the 
animal  has  been  allowed  to  live  a  sufficient  time  for  these  sub- 
sidiary events  to  have  cleared  away,  leaving  only  what  we  have 
called  the  '  deficiency '  phenomena,  due  to  the  loss  of  the  cortical 
area  alone.  On  the  other  hand  it  must  be  remembered  that  the 
movements  of  the  monkey  are  more  intricate  in  origin,  more 
'  skilled '  than  those  of  the  dog ;  and  it  may  be  that  differences 
in  the  characters  of  movements  determine  the  possibility  of  their 
recovery.  In  illustration  of  this  we  may  quote  the  experience 
that,  after  the  removal  of  the  arm  area  in  the  monkey,  a  certain 
awkwardness  in  the  movements  of  the  thumb  is  one  of  the  last 
effects  of  the  operation. 

§  659.  Before  we  proceed  however  any  further  in  the  dis- 
cussion, it  will  be  of  advantage  to  turn  aside  to  what  is  known 
concerning  the  cortical  motor  region  in  man.  As  we  have  already 
said,  theoretical  considerations  lead  us  to  believe  that  the  cortical 
motor  region  in  man  is  disposed  in  accordance  with  the  plan  of 
the  anthropoid  ape  as  ascertained  experimentally,  but  with  the 
differentiation  carried  still  further;  and  the  few  cases  of  experi- 
mental stimulation  of  the  human  cortex  support  this  view.  Our 
chief  knowledge  in  this  matter  is  derived  from  the  study  of 
disease;  and  in  this,  the  advantages  of  dealing  with  one  of 
ourselves  are  largely  counterbalanced  by  the  disadvantages  due 
to  disease  being  so  often  anatomically  diffuse  and  physiologically 
changeful  and  progressive. 

We  said  above  that  during  experiments  on  animals  stimulation 
of  any  part  of  the  motor  region  may  under  abnormal  conditions 
lead  to  general  epileptiform  convulsions.  Now  clinical  study  has 
shewn  that  in  man  certain  kinds  of  epileptic  attacks  are  of 
similar  cortical  origin.  In  these  cases  it  has  been  observed  that 
the  attack  begins  in  a  particular  movement,  by  contractions  of 
particular  muscles,  or  of  the  muscles  of  a  particular  region  of  the 
body,  of  the  hand,  foot,  toe,  thumb,  &c.,  and  then  spreads  in  a 
definite  order  or  '  march '  over  the  muscles  of  other  regions  until 
the  whole  body  is  involved.  When  in  an  experiment  on  an 
animal  epileptiform  convulsions  supervene,  they  similarly  start 
from  the  region  of  the  body,  the  motor  area  of  which  ia  beneath 
the  electrodes  at  the  time,  and  similarly  spread  by  a  definite 
'  march '  over  the  whole  body.  Hence  in  the  human  epileptiform 
attacks  of  which  we  are  speaking,  it  has  been  inferred  that  the 
immediate  exciting  cause  of  the  attack  is  to  be  sought  in  events 
taking  place  in  that  part  of  the  cortex  which  serves  as  the  area 
for  the  movement  which  ushers  in  the  attack.  Further  inquiry 
has  not  only  confirmed  this  view,  but  has  also  shewn  that  the 
topography  of  the  cortical  areas  in  man,  as  thus  determined, 
very  closely  follows  that  of  the  monkey. 

Other  diseases  of  the  cortex  have  been  marked,  among  other 


CHAP,  ii.]  THE   BRAIN.  1053 

symptoms,  by  loss  or  impairment  of  particular  movements.  In 
most  of  such  cases,  the  cortical  lesion  has  been  of  such  an  extent 
as  to  involve  a  number  of  special  areas  at  the  same  time,  and  so  to 
lead  to  loss  or  impairment  of  movement  over  relatively  considerable 
regions  of  the  body,  such  as  the  whole  of  one  arm ;  and  in  general 
the  teaching  of  these  cases  of  disease,  while  confirming  the 
deductions  from  the  monkey,  and  giving  us  some  general  idea  of 
the  topography  of  the  human  motor  cortical  region,  has  at  present 
given  us  approximate  results  only.  Figs.  131  and  132  shew  in 
broad  diagrammatic  manner  the  position  and  relative  extent  of 
the  motor  areas  for  the  leg,  arm  and  face  in  man,  as  far  as  has 
yet  been  ascertained.  To  assist  the  reader  we  give  at  the  same 
time  diagrams  Figs.  129,  130  illustrating  the  nomenclature  of 
the  surface  of  the  human  brain. 

One  area  is  of  special  and  instructive  interest.  Speech  is 
an  eminently  'skilled'  movement.  We  have  seen  that  in  the 
monkey  the  area  for  the  mouth  and  tongue  lies  at  the  ventral  end 
of  the  central  fissure  or  fissure  of  Rolando,  ventral  to  the  arm 
area,  and  that  the  extreme  ventral  and  front  part  of  the  motor 
region  just  above  the  fissure  of  Sylvius  supplies  an  area  which 
we  marked  as  that  of  phonation  (Fig.  126).  In  the  monkey  the 
area  of  phonation  is  determined  by  experimental  stimulation ;  in 
man,  in  a  similar  position,  on  the  third  or  lowest  frontal  con- 
volution, sometimes  called  Broca's  convolution,  ventral  to  and  in 
front  of,  and  probably  overlapping  backwards  the  area  which  in 
Fig.  131  is  marked  '  face '  and  which  includes  the  mouth  and 
tongue,  clinical  study  has  disclosed  the  existence  of  an  area  which 
may  be  spoken  of  as  the  area  of  '  speech.'  Lesions  of  the  cortex 
in  this  area  cause  a  loss  of  or  interference  with  speech,  the 
condition  being  known  as  aphasia ;  to  this  we  shall  presently 
return.  In  Fig.  131  this  area  is  shewn  in  an  approximate 
manner. 

The  movements  of  speech  are  essentially  bilateral  movements. 
In  the  dog  and  monkey  various  bilateral  movements  may  be 
excited  by  stimulation  of  the  appropriate  area  in  either  hemi- 
sphere; and  analogy  would  lead  us  to  suppose  that  in  man,  the 
movements  of  speech  would  be  connected  with  the  speech  area 
in  both  one  and  the  other  hemisphere.  The  results  of  lesions 
however  shew  that  it  is  in  most  cases  especially  the  left  hemi- 
sphere which  is  connected  with  speech  ;  it  is  a  lesion  in  the  third 
frontal  convolution  of  the  left  hemisphere,  often  associated  with 
other  lesions  of  the  same  hemisphere  leading  to  paralysis  of  the 
right  side  of  the  body  and  face,  which  causes  aphasia,  it  being 
only  in  exceptional  cases  that  the  condition  results  from  a  lesion 
of  the  corresponding  area  of  cortex  on  the  right  hemisphere. 

In  man,  then,  clinical  study  corroborates  the  conclusions  de- 
duced from  the  experimental  investigation  of  the  dog  and  of  the 
monkey,  but  still  leaves  us  in  uncertainty  as  to  the  question  what, 


1054  CORTICAL   MOTOR   REGION   IN   MAN.     [BOOK  m. 

FRO    NT    A 


Par.-Oc. 


L  0  B 


FIG.  129.     DIAGRAM  or  THE  GYRI  (CONVOLUTIONS)  SULCI,  (FISSURES)  ON  THE 

LATERAL    SURFACE    OF    THE    RlGHT    HEMISPHERE    OF    MAN.       (GowerS.) 


F.  Rolando 


FIG.  130.     THE  SAME  ON  THE  MESIAL  SURFACE.     (Gowers.) 

In  both  figures  the  sulci  are  indicated  by  italic  and  the  convolutions  by 
roman  type. 

The  following  list  of  some  synonyms  may  perhaps  be  of  use  in  connection  with 
these  figures  and  those  of  the  brain  of  the  monkey,  Figs.  126,  127. 

Gyri,  or  Convolutions.  Precentral  or  anterior  central  =  ascending  frontal. 
Postcentral  or  posterior  central  =  ascending  parietal.  Superior  temporal  =  infra- 
marginal  =  first  temporal.  Triangular  lobule  =  cuneus.  Central  lobe  =  Island  of 
Eeil.  Paracentral  lobule  =  the  mesial  face  of  the  ascending  frontal,  within  the 
marginal  gyrus.  Cingulum  =  the  part  of  the  gyrus  fornicatus  which  adjoins  the 
Corpus  callosum.  Gyrus  Hippocampi  =  uncinate  gyrus,  though  the  latter  name  is 
sometimes  restricted  to  the  front  part  of  the  hippocampal  gyrus;  the  two  may  be 
considered  as  a  continuation  of  the  gyrus  fornicatus,  and  the  three  together, 
forming  a  series,  have  been  called  "the  great  limbic  lobe." 

Sulci  or  Fissures.  Central  —  Eolandic,  or  of  Rolando.  Perpendicular  =  parieto- 
•  occipital.  Parietal  =  intraparietal  or  sometimes  interparietal. 

Temporo-sphenoidal  lobe  =  temporal  lobe. 


CHAP,  ii.] 


THE   BRAIN. 


1055 


Fr.L 


Te.C 


FIG.  131.     THE  LATERAL  SURFACE  OF  THE  EIGHT  CEREBRAL  HEMISPHERE  OF 
IN  OUTLINE,  TO  ILLUSTRATE  THE  CORTICAL  AREAS.     Reduced  from  nature. 

The  position  of  the  areas  of  the  cortex  concerned  with  movements  of  the  face,  arm, 
and  leg,  and  with  the  senses  of  sight  and  hearing  are  approximately  shewn. 
The  position  of  the  area  connected  with  speech  (Broca's  centre)  is  also  shewn 
for  the  sake  of  comparison  of  it  with  the  position  of  the  other  areas ;  the 
representation  of  speech  in  the  cortex  cerebri  lies  however  in  the  left  hemisphere 
chiefly. 


Oc 


.  L.  Occipital  lobe;  Fr.  L.  Frontal  lobe;  Te.  L.  Temporal  lobe;  Sy.f.  the  fissure 
of  Sylvius ;  C.  f.  the  central  fissure  (Eolandic) ;  Cm.  f.  indicates  the  position 
of  the  posterior  end  of  the  calloso-marginal  fissure. 


Fr.L 


Oc.L 


Te.L 


FIG.  132.     THE  MESIAL  SURFACE  OF  THE  EIGHT  CEREBRAL  HEMISPHERE  OF  MAN 
IN  OUTLINE,  TO  ILLUSTRATE  THE  CORTICAL  AREAS. 

The  areas  shown  are  those  connected  with  the  movements  of  the  leg,  and  with  the 
senses  of  sight  and  smell. 

Fr.  L.  the  frontal  pole  of  the  hemisphere ;  Oc.  L.  the  occipital  pole,  Te.  L.  the 
temporal  pole.  Cm.  f.  the  calloso-marginal  fissure  separating  the  marginal 
gyrus  above  from  the  gyrus  fornicatus  below.  Cf.  marks  the  situation  of 
the  central  fissure,  the  fissure  itself  not  being  apparent  on  the  mesial  aspect 
of  the  hemisphere.  The  corpus  callosum  and  the  anterior  commissure  are 
seen  in  cross  section. 


F. 


67 


1056  VOLUNTARY   MOVEMENTS.  [BOOK  in. 

and  what  alone  are  the  absolutely  permanent  effects  of  the  loss  of 
a  cortical  area  and  nothing  else.  On  the  one  hand,  in  the  cases 
in  which  recovery  of  a  movement  follows  upon  its  loss  or  impair- 
ment, it  is  open  for  us  to  suppose  that  the  lesion  itself  was 
temporary,  and  that  with  the  cure  of  the  malady  the  cortical  area 
regained  its  normal  condition.  On  the  other  hand,  where  the 
disease  continues,  the  permanency  of  the  loss  of  any  movement 
may  be  attributed  to  the  disease  doing  more  than  merely  suspend 
the  function  of  the  cortical  area.  Aphasia,  especially  in  young 
persons,  has  been  followed  by  recovery,  but  in  such  cases  it  has 
been  supposed  that  the  dormant  area  on  the  right  side  has  been 
awakened  to  activity  by  the  loss  of  the  left  area ;  and  in  support 
of  this  view  cases  have  been  recorded  in  which  a  first  aphasia,  due 
to  a  lesion  on  the  left  side,  has  been  followed  by  a  second  aphasia 
due  to  a  sequent  lesion  occurring  on  the  right  side.  On  the 
whole  perhaps  the  evidence  of  clinical  study  tends  to  shew  that 
in  man  the  loss  of  movement  due  to  the  destruction  by  disease 
of  an  area  is  a  permanent  one,  though  actual  demonstration  of 
this  is  wanting. 

§  660.  We  may  now  return  to  the  discussion  of  the  question, 
what  is  the  part  played  by  a  motor  area,  and  by  the  contribution 
from  that  area  to  the  pyramidal  tract  in  carrying  out  the  move- 
ments with  which  the  area  is  associated  ? 

We  may  premise  that  the  evidence  points  very  distinctly  to 
the  conclusion  that  whatever  be  the  nature  of  the  whole  chain  of 
events  of  which  the  cortical  area  seems  to  be  a  sort  of  centre,  the 
fibres  of  the  pyramidal  tract  serve  as  the  channel  of  processes 
which  we  must  regard  as  efferent  in  nature.  It  is  perfectly  true 
that  in  many  cases  at  least  the  removal  of  a  cortical  area  has  led 
to  diminished  sensibility  of  the  part  in  which  movements  are 
excited  by  stimulation  of  the  area ;  and  there  are  many  facts,  of 
which  we  shall  presently  quote  a  very  striking  one,  which  go  to 
shew  that  the  cortex  of  the  motor  region  is  largely  influenced  by 
sensory  impulses  from  various  parts  of  the  body ;  but  we  cannot 
suppose  that  the  pyramidal  tract  is  the  channel  by  which  such 
sensory  impulses  reach  the  cortex.  As  we  have  previously  (§  568) 
urged,  the  fact  that  the  degeneration  of  the  fibres  in  the  tract  is 
a  descending  one,  cannot  be  trusted  by  itself  to  prove  that  the 
direction  in  which  the  fibres  carry  impulses  is  only  that  from  the 
cortex  downwards ;  but  this  added  to  the  fact  that  when  the  fibres 
of  the  tract  are  stimulated  at  any  part  of  their  course,  movements, 
the  signs  of  the  occurrence  of  efferent  centrifugal  impulses,  are 
produced,  leaves  no  doubt  that  the  tract  is  one  of  efferent  fibres. 
Hence  we  may  infer  that  whatever  be  the  nature  of  the  events 
taking  place  in  a  motor  area  during  the  carrying  out  of  a  move- 
ment, the  part  played  by  the  fibres  of  the  pyramidal  tract  is  that 
of  carrying  efferent  impulses  from  the  area  to  the  muscles  con- 
cerned. 


CHAP.  IL]  THE   BRAIN.  1057 

Let  us  consider  first  the  movements  of  speech  in  man,  the 
evidence  touching  the  connection  of  which  with  an  area  on  the 
third  frontal  convolution  appears  so  very  clear.  Speech  is 
eminently  a  '  skilled'  movement ;  it  involves  the  most  delicate 
coordination  of  several  muscular  contractions,  and  we  may  certainly 
say  of  it  that  it  has  to  be  '  learnt.'  The  whole  chain  of  co- 
ordinated events  by  which  the  utterance  of  a  sentence,  a  word,  or 
any  vocal  sign  is  accomplished  consists  of  many  links,  the  breaking 
of  any  of  which  will  lead  to  failure  of  one  kind  or  another  in  the 
act.  Something  may  go  wrong  in  the  glossal  or  other  muscles,  in 
the  nerve  endings  in  those  muscles,  or  in  the  fibres  of  the  nerves, 
hypoglossal  and  others,  between  the  central  nervous  system  and 
the  muscles,  or  something  may  go  wrong  in  that  part  of  the 
central  nervous  system,  the  bulb  to  wit,  in  which  a  certain  amount 
of  coordination  is  carried  out  just  previous  to  the  issue  of  the 
motor  impulses.  Damage  done  to  any  of  these  parts  of  the 
mechanism  may  lead  to  dumbness  or  to  imperfect  speech.  In  the 
latter  case  the  imperfections  have  a  certain  character ;  if  we  are 
at  all  able  to  gather  the  wish  of  the  speaker,  we  recognize  that  he 
is  attempting  to  utter  the  right  words  in  the  right  sequence,  but 
that  his  efforts  are  frustrated  by  imperfect  coordination  or  imperfect 
muscular  action;  his  speech  is  'thick,'  the  syllables  are  blurred 
and  the  like.  Disease  of  the  bulb  at  times  leads  to  imperfect 
speech  of  this  kind  in  which  the  imperfection  may  be  recognized 
as  due  to  the  lack  of  proper  coordination  of  motor  impulses.  The 
affection  of  speech,  known  as  'aphasia,'  which  is  caused  by 
lesions  of  the  cortex  is  of  a  different  character,  and  the  forms 
of  imperfect  speech  caused  by  bulbar  disease  have  justly  been 
distinguished  from  true  aphasia  by  the  use  of  other  terms.  Cases 
of  complete  aphasia  in  which  all  power  of  speech  is  lost,  do  little 
more  than  help  us  to  ascertain  the  topographical  position  in  the 
cortex  of  the  'speech'  area,  but  cases  of  partial  aphasia  are 
especially  instructive.  Without  attempting  to  go  into  the  details 
of  the  subject  and  into  the  many  considerations  which  have  to 
be  had  in  mind  in  dealing  with  it,  for  there  are  different  kinds 
of  aphasia,  we  may  venture  to  say  that  the  striking  feature  of 
partial  aphasia  is  the  failure  to  say  certain  words  or  syllables, 
and  the  tendency  to  substitute  some  wrong  word  or  syllable  for 
the  right  one.  The  words  or  syllables  which  are  uttered  are 
rightly  pronounced  without  defect  of  articulation;  and  in  many 
cases,  though  the  right  word  cannot  be  produced  as  a  direct 
effort  of  the  will,  it  may  be  uttered  under  the  influence  of  an 
emotion,  or  indeed  sometimes  as  the  result  of  some  psychical 
processes  more  complex  than  those  involved  in  the  mere  voli- 
tional effort  to  say  the  word.  An  instructive  case  is  recorded  of 
a  man  suffering  from  slight  aphasia,  who  after  several  failures  to 
say  the  word  'no'  by  itself,  at  last  said,  "I  can't  say  no,  sir." 

From  the  phenomena  of  partial  aphasia  we  may  draw  the 

67—2 


1058  VOLUNTARY   MOVEMENTS.  [BOOK  in. 

deduction  that  the  cortical  speech  area  does  not  carry  out  the 
whole  of  the  coordination  of  the  impulses  involved  in  articulation. 
That  coordination  is  exceedingly  complex,  and  we  ought  perhaps 
to  recognize  in  it  more  than  one  degree  or  kind  of  coordination. 
The  failure  of  articulation  in  disease  of  the  bulb  shews  that  a 
certain  amount  of  coordination  takes  place  there;  for  the  affec- 
tions of  speech  due  to  bulbar  disease  are  not  the  same  as  those 
resulting  from  the  mere  loss  of  this  or  that  muscle  or  nerve.  We 
must  of  course  admit  that  some,  possibly  a  great  deal,  of  coordi- 
nation of  a  certain  kind  takes  place  in  the  cortex,  for  the  bulb 
cannot  by  itself  be  made  to  speak ;  exactly  how  much,  the 
knowledge  at  present  at  our  disposal  leaves  a  matter  of  great 
uncertainty;  but  it  is  sufficient  for  our  present  purpose  to 
recognize  that  whatever  may  be  the  nature  of  the  events  taking 
place  in  the  cortical  area  during  the  act  of  speech,  those  events 
make  use  of  the  machinery  already  provided  in  the  bulb.  The 
word  spoken  does  not  start,  so  to  speak,  ready  made  in  the  cortex ; 
it  is  not  that  a  group  of  impulses  start  from  the  cortex  with  their 
coordination  fully  achieved,  and  pass  along  certain  nerve  fibres  to 
certain  muscles,  making  their  way  without  change  through  the 
tangle  of  the  bulb,  as  if  this  were  merely  a  bundle  of  lines 
offering  paths  for,  but  exercising  no  influence  over  the  impulses. 
We  must  rather  suppose  that  something  takes  place  in  the  cortex 
of  the  third  frontal  convolution,  as  the  result  of  which  efferent 
impulses  pass  along  the  appropriate  fibres  of  the  pyramidal  tract 
to  the  bulb,  and  there  start  a  series  of  events  leading  to  the  issue 
of  the  coordinated  impulses  by  which  the  word  is  spoken. 

§  661.  We  have  no  reason  whatever  to  think  that  the  cortical 
area  for  speech  differs  in  its  fundamental  characters  from  other 
divisions  of  the  motor  region,  and  are  justified  in  carrying  on  to 
other  areas  the  deduction  we  have  just  drawn  in  connection  with 
the  speech  area.  With  that  end  in  view  we  may  now  turn  back 
to  the  experimental  results  obtained  on  the  dog,  and  it  will  make 
our  discussion  simpler  if  we  take  as  an  illustration  some  large  area 
such  as  the  forelimb  area. 

We  have  seen  that  stimulation  of  this  area  produces  what  we 
may,  to  start  with,  speak  of  simplyas  movements  of  the  fore-limb;  and 
guided  by  the  analogy  of  speech  in  man  we  may  confidently  conclude 
that  when  the  dog  voluntarily  moves  the  fore-limb,  the  act  is  earned 
out  by  means  of  events  taking  place  in  the  fore-limb  cortical  area. 
The  simplicity  of  the  electrical  phenomena  resulting  from  cortical 
stimulation,  which  we  described  in  §  657,  might  at  first  sight  lead 
us  to  conclude  that  the  whole  matter  was  fairly  simple;  and  indeed 
some  writers  appear  to  entertain  the  conception  that  in  a  voluntary 
movement  such  as  that  of  the  fore-limb,  all  that  takes  place  is  that 
the  '  will'  stimulates  certain  cells  in  the  cortical  area  causing  the 
discharge  of  motor  impulses  along  the  pyramidal  fibres  connected 
with  those  cells,  and  that  these  motor  impulses  travel  straight  down 


CHAP.  IL]  THE   BRAIN.  1059 

the  pyramidal  tract  to  the  motor  fibres  of  the  appropriate  nerves, 
undergoing  possibly  some  change  at  the  place  in  the  cord  where 
the  pyramidal  fibre  makes  junction  with  the  fibre  of  the  anterior 
root,  but  deriving  their  chief  if  not  their  whole  coordination  from 
the  cortex  itself,  that  is  to  say,  being  coordinated  at  their  very 
starting  point.  That  such  a  view  is  untenable,  and  that  the 
simplicity  of  the  electrical  phenomena  is  misleading  is  shewn  by 
the  following  two  considerations  among  others.  On  the  one  hand, 
as  was  shewn  in  a  previous  section,  the  coordination  of  movements 
may  be  carried  out  apart  from  the  cortex,  namely,  in  the  absence 
of  the  hemispheres ;  and  we  can  hardly  suppose  that  there  should 
be  two  quite  distinct  systems  of  coordination  to  carry  out  the  same 
movement,  one  employed  when  volition  was  the  moving  cause,  and 
the  other  when  something  else  led  to  the  movement.  On  the 
other  hand,  the  analogy  of  speech  justifies  us  in  concluding  that 
the  cortical  processes  do  take  advantage  of  a  coordination  effected 
by  the  action  of  other  parts  of  the  nervous  system. 

Bearing  this  in  mind,  we  may  recall  attention  to  the  remarkable 
effects  which  result  from  removal  of  the  area.  These  are  twofold. 
In  the  first  place,  there  is  more  or  less  complete  paralysis  of  the 
limb ;  all  the  movements  of  the  limb  are  for  a  time  ineffective.  It 
is  not  that  purely  voluntary  movements  are  alone,  so  to  speak,  cut 
out,  the  reflex  and  other  movements  are  also  impaired  or  tem- 
porarily abolished,  and  as  we  have  already  said  in  many  cases  at 
least  the  sensations  of  the  limb  are  interfered  with.  These  troubles 
are  of  course  in  part  the  effects  of  the  mere  operative  interference 
belonging  to  what  we  spoke  of  in  §  582,  as  being  of  the  nature  of 
shock.  But,  even  giving  full  weight  to  this  consideration,  there 
remains  the  fact  that  the  cortical  area  is  associated  with  the  various 
coordinating  and  other  nervous  mechanisms  belonging  to  the  limb 
by  such  close  ties  that  these  are  thrown  into  disorder  when  it  is 
injured.  And  side  by  side  with  this  we  may  put  the  remarkable 
fact  previously  stated,  that  during  an  abnormal  condition  of  the 
cortical  area  stimulation  of  the  area,  instead  of  producing  the 
appropriate  movements  confined  to  the  limb,  may  give  rise  to 
movements  of  other  parts  culminating  in  epileptiform  con- 
vulsions. 

In  the  second  place,  this  paralysis  is  temporary  only,  the 
voluntary  movements  are  after  a  while  regained,  and  that  in  spite 
of  the  fore-limb  moiety  of  the  pyramidal  tract  permanently  de- 
generating along  its  whole  length,  neither  it  nor  the  cortical  area 
ever  being  regenerated.  This  shews  that  whatever  be  the  chain 
of  events  in  the  intact  animal,  it  is  possible  for  the  *  will '  of  the 
animal  to  get  at  the  muscles  and  motor  mechanisms  of  the  fore-limb 
by  some  other  path  than  that  provided  by  the  appropriate  cortical 
area  and  corresponding  path  of  the  pyramidal  tract ;  and  the  facts 
previously  recorded  (§  658)  shew  that  that  other  part  is  not  the 
corresponding  part  of  the  pyramidal  system  belonging  to  the  other 


1060  VOLUNTARY   MOVEMENTS.  [BOOK  m. 

half  of  the  hemisphere  and  indeed  is  not  any  part  at  all  of  the 
whole  pyramidal  system.  The  '  will/  whatever  be  the  processes  by 
which  it  takes  origin,  and  wherever  be  the  place  where  they  are 
carried  on,  is  able  in  the  absence  of  the  pyramidal  system  to 
produce  its  effect  on  the  motor  fibres  of  the  brachial  nerves  by 
working  on  other  parts  of  the  central  nervous  system. 

Hence  while  admitting  as  we  must  do  that  in  the  intact  animal 
the  cortical  area  and  pyramidal  tract  play  their  part  in  carrying  out 
voluntary  movements,  their  action  is  not  of  that  simple  character 
supposed  by  the  view  referred  to  above.  On  the  contrary,  we  are 
driven  to  regard  them  rather  as  links,  important  links  it  is  true, 
but  still  links,  in  a  complex  chain.  As  we  have  already  urged,  we 
may  probably  speak  of  the  changes  taking  place  in  the  pyramidal 
fibres  as  being  on  the  whole  of  the  nature  of  efferent  impulses; 
but  we  should  be  going  beyond  the  evidence  if  we  concluded  that 
they  were  identical  with  the  ordinary  efferent  impulses  of  motor 
nerves.  And  above  all  it  must  not  be  left  unnoticed  that  the  cortical 
area  has  close  if  not  direct  connections  of  a  sensory  nature  with  the 
part  in  whose  movements  it  is  concerned.  This  is  shewn  by  the 
following  remarkable  results  which  may  make  their  appearance 
when  stimulation  of  the  cortex  is  carried  on  while  the  animal  (dog)  is 
in  a  particular  stage  of  the  influence  of  morphia.  If  a  subminimal 
stimulus  be  found,  that  is  a  current  of  such  intensity  that  applied 
to  a  motor  area  it  will  produce  no  movement,  but  if  increased  ever 
so  slightly  will  give  a  feeble  contraction  of  the  appropriate  muscles, 
it  may  be  observed  that  a  slight  stimulus,  such  as  gently  stroking 
the  skin  over  the  muscles  in  question,  will  render  the  previous 
subminimal  stimulus  effective  and  so  call  forth  a  movement. 
Thus  if  the  area  experimented  on  be  that  connected  with  the 
lifting  of  the  forepaw,  and  the  subminimal  stimulus  be  applied 
to  the  area  at  intervals,  after  several  applications  followed  by  no 
movements,  a  gentle  stroke  or  two  over  the  skin  of  the  paw  will 
lead  to  the  paw  being  lifted  the  next  time  the  stimulus  is  applied 
to  the^urea.  A  similar  result,  but  less  sure  and  striking,  may 
upon  the  stimulation  of  parts  of  the  body  other  than  the 
part  corresponding  to  the  area  stimulated.  Then  again  it  has 
been  observed  that  in  certain  other  stages  of  the  influence  of 
morphia,  the  cortex  and  the  rest  of  the  nervous  system  are  in 
such  a  condition  that  the  application  of  even  a  momentary 
stimulus  to  an  area  leads  not  to  a  simple  movement  but  to  a  long- 
continued  tonic  contraction  of  the  appropriate  muscles.  Under 
these  circumstances,  a  gentle  stimulus,  such  as  stroking  the  skin, 
or  blowing  on  the  face,  applied  immediately  after  the  application 
of  the  electric  stimulus  to  the  area,  suddenly  cuts  short  the 
contraction,  and  brings  the  muscles  at  once  to  rest  and  normal 
flaccidity. 

These  experiments  shew  that  the  development  of  the  processes 
in  the  cortex  leading  to  the  issue  of  what  we  have  agreed  to  call 


CHAP.  IL]  THE   BRAIN.  1061 

efferent  impulses  along  the  pyramidal  fibres  is  markedly  affected 
by  sensory  impulses  and  especially  by  sensory  impulses  started  in 
the  skin  overlying  and  corresponding  to  the  muscles  put  into 
movement.  How  those  sensory  impulses  reach  the  cortex  we  do 
not  exactly  know ;  but  we  have  no  evidence  to  shew  that  afferent, 
centripetal  impulses  can  travel  backwards  so  to  speak  along  the 
pyramidal  fibres;  and  it  is  more  reasonable  to  suppose  that  the 
sensory  impulses  in  question  reach  the  cortex  by  the  ordinary  paths 
of  sensory  impulses,  which  we  shall  presently  discuss.  We  may 
therefore  take  the  results  of  the  experiments  as  shewing  how  close 
is  the  connection  of  the  motor  area  with  the  sensory  mechanisms  of 
the  spinal  cord  and  lower  parts  of  the  brain,  and  as  illustrating  the 
complexity  of  the  chain  of  events  by  which  the  motor  area  brings 
about  voluntary  movements. 

§  662.  We  have  above  used  the  general  phrase  'movements  of 
the  limb,'  since  in  the  dog  it  is  not  easy  to  pick  out  certain 
movements  as  being  particularly  skilled  movements.  In  the 
monkey  such  a  distinction  is  easier.  In  this  animal,  as  we  have 
said,  recovery  of  voluntary  movement  also  takes  place  after  removal 
of  a  cortical  area,  or  at  least  has  done  so  in  many  cases ;  and  while 
the  phenomena  immediately  following  removal  on  the  whole 
resemble  those  witnessed  in  the  dog,  a  certain  order  of  recovery 
may  be  observed;  the  more  skilled  movements  are  the  last  to 
return.  When  for  instance  the  arm  area  is  removed,  the  delicate 
movements  of  the  hand,  of  the  thumb  and  finger,  are  the  last  to 
be  re-established ;  and  a  condition  of  things  may  be  met  with  in 
which  the  animal  after  removal,  say  of  the  arm  area  in  the  left 
hemisphere,  uses  by  preference  the  left  hand  at  a  time  when,  if 
prevented  from  using  that  hand,  he  is  able  to  use  the  right ;  that 
is  to  say,  the  recovery  in  the  right  limb  after  the  removal  of  the 
area  on  the  left  side  is  nearly  but  not  quite  complete ;  the  '  will ' 
can  gain  access  to  the  right  hand,  but  not  so  easily  as  to  the  left 
hand,  and  this  latter  is  used,  though  under  ordinary  circumstances 
it  would  not  be  used. 

When  we  turn  to  man,  in  whom  the  great  development  of  the 
pyramidal  system  and  differentiation  of  the  cortical  area  is  paral- 
leled by  the  prominence  of  skilled  and  trained  movements,  the 
analogy  of  the  phenomena  of  speech,  if  it  be  true  as  clinical 
histories  seem  to  shew  that  destruction  by  disease  of  the  speech 
area  of  both  sides  causes  permanent  aphasia,  would  lead  us  to 
conclude  that  at  least  highly  skilled  voluntary  movements  are 
carried  out  by  the  pyramidal  system  and  by  that  alone.  But  in 
reference  to  this  it  must  be  remembered  that  such  a  permanent 
aphasia  may  be  due,  not  to  mere  loss  of  the  pyramidal  channel, 
not  to  the  will  being  merely  unable  to  gain  access  to  lower 
coordinating  mechanisms,  but  to  the  absence  of  the  differentiated 
cortical  grey  matter,  by  reason  of  which  absence  the  will  cannot 
initiate  the  first  processes  of  the  act  of  speech ;  it  may  be  that 


1062  VOLUNTARY   MOVEMENTS.  [BOOK  in. 

were  it  able  to  do  so,  the  processes  so  started  might  in  the  absence 
of  the  pyramidal  tract,  find  some  other  way  to  the  bulbar  mechan- 
ism as  in  the  case  of  the  unskilled  movements  of  the  dog.  This 
point  however  clinical  histories  have  not  definitely  settled.  Moreover 
in  dealing  with  the  phenomena  of  the  nervous  system  of  man  as 
revealed  by  disease,  we  meet  in  reference  to  the  cerebral  cortex 
the  same  difficulty  that  we  dwelt  upon  in  dealing  with  the  spinal 
cord  (§  591).  Lesions  of  the  pyramidal  system,  of  the  internal 
capsule  for  instance,  lead  to  the  loss  not  only  of  skilled  but  of  all 
voluntary  movements ;  according  to  the  character  and  position  of 
the  lesion  this  or  that  part  of  the  body  is  wholly  withdrawn  from 
the  influence  of  the  will.  And  it  is  possible  to  maintain  the  thesis 
that  man  has  become  so  developed  as  to  his  nervous  system  and 
the  motor  cortex,  so  accustomed  to  make  use  exclusively  of  the 
pyramidal  system  that  the  will  has  lost  the  power,  still  possessed 
by  lower  animals,  to  gain  access  by  some  path  other  than  the 
pyramidal  one  to  the  immediate  nervous  mechanisms  of  movement. 
The  data  for  forming  a  satisfactory  conclusion  as  to  this  point  are 
so  few  and  uncertain  that  it  would  be  unprofitable  to  discuss  the 
question  here ;  but  we  may  venture  to  point  out  that,  great  as  is  the 
development  of  the  cerebral  cortex  and  the  pyramidal  system  in 
man,  that  development  is  accompanied  by  a  hardly  less  striking 
expansion  of  other  parts  of  the  brain  not  directly  connected  with 
the  pyramidal  system  which  we  have  previously  seen  reason  to 
associate  with  the  coordination  of  movements,  for  example  the 
cerebellum.  And  indeed  it  is  clear  that,  admitting  the  pyramidal 
tract  to  be  the  ordinary  channel  by  which  volitional  impulses  pass 
to,  or  by  which  the  will  gains  access  to,  the  motor  mechanisms 
immediately  associated  with  the  anterior  roots  of  this  or  that  spinal 
nerve,  we  must  also  admit  that  those  volitional  impulses  passing 
along  the  pyramidal  tract,  or  at  least  some  of  the  processes  con- 
stituting the  will,  are  in  connection  with,  and  thus  are  influenced 
by  the  condition  of,  other  parts  of  the  brain.  When  for  instance 
a  gymnast  executes  a  skilled  voluntary  movement  in  which  all  his 
four  limbs  and  other  parts  as  well  perhaps  of  his  body  are  involved, 
it  is  probably  the  case  that  changes  of  the  nature  of  efferent 
impulses  sweep  down  his  pyramidal  tract,  and  that  these  impulses, 
starting  in  a  definite  order  from  his  cortex,  that  is  to  say  having 
undergone  a  certain  amount  of  initial  coordination  at  their  very 
origin,  meet  with  further  coordination  in  the  spinal  grey  matter, 
which  serves  as  a  set  of  nuclei  of  origin  for  the  motor  nerves  con- 
cerned in  the  movement,  before  they  issue  as  ordinary  motor 
impulses  along  the  anterior  roots.  But  this  is  not  all.  Should  the 
gymnast's  semicircular  canals  happen  to  be  injured  and  his  cere- 
bellum thereby  be  troubled,  or  mischief  fall  on  some  other  part 
of  the  brain  which  like  this  has  no  direct  connection  with  either 
the  pyramidal  tract  or  the  motor  cortex,  the  movement  fails 
through  lack  of  coordination,  though  both  the  cortex,  the  pyra- 


CHAP,  ii.]  THE   BRAIN.  1063 

midal  tract,  and  the  spinal  motor  mechanisms  remain  as  they 
were  before.  Obviously  the  carrying  out  of  a  voluntary  move- 
ment is  a  very  complex  proceeding,  and  the  motor  cortex  with 
the  pyramidal  tract  is  only  one  part  of  the  whole  mechanism  ;  so 
far  from  the  whole  business  being  confined  to  these  it  is  perhaps 
no  exaggeration  to  say  that  in  each  movement  of  the  kind  most 
parts  of  the  whole  brain  have  a  greater  or  less  share. 

The  exact  nature  of  the  part  played  by  the  cortex  and  the 
pyramidal  tract  in  voluntary  movements  our  present  knowledge  is 
inadequate  to  define.  When  we  pass  in  review  a  series  of  brains 
from  the  lower  to  the  higher  and  see  how  the  pyramidal  system 
is  so  to  speak  grafted  on  to  the  rest  of  the  brain,  when  we  observe 
how  the  increasing  differentiation  of  the  motor  cortex  runs  parallel 
to  the  increasing  possession  of  skilled  educated  movements,  we  may 
perhaps  suppose  that  '  a  short  cut '  from  the  cortex  to  the  origins 
of  the  several  motor  nerves,  such  as  is  afforded  by  the  pyramidal 
fibres,  from  the  advantages  it  offers  to  the  more  primitive  path 
from  segment  to  segment  along  the  cerebrospinal  axis  has  by 
natural  selection  been  developed  into  being  in  man  the  chief  and 
most  important  instrument  for  carrying  out  voluntary  movements; 
but,  we  repeat,  it  remains  even  in  its  highest  development  a  link 
in  a  chain,  and  a  knowledge  of  how  the  whole  chain  works  is  at 
present  hidden  from  us. 

We  must  not  here  wander  into  psychological  problems,  but 
may  repeat  that  in  the  above  discussion  we  have  used  the  word 
1  will '  in  a  general  sense  only.  A  man  may  be  brought  into  a 
condition,  for  instance  in  certain  hypnotic  phases,  in  which  he  can 
carry  out  all  the  various  skilled  movements  which  he  has  inherited 
or  which  he  has  learnt ;  and  yet,  according  to  some  definitions  of 
the  word  '  will,'  those  movements  could  not  be  said  to  be  initiated 
by  his  will.  It  can  hardly  be  doubted  that  in  such  cases  the 
motor  cortex  and  pyramidal  tract  play  their  usual  part.  But  we 
may  pass  from  such  cases  as  these  through  others,  until  we  come 
to  cases  where  a  skilled  movement  which  has  been  learnt  and 
practised  by  the  working  of  an  intelligent  will,  may  continue  to 
be  carried  out  under  circumstances  which  seem  to  preclude  the 
intervention  of  any  conscious  will  at  all ;  and  the  transition  from 
one  case  to  another  is  so  gradual,  that  it  is  impossible  to  suppose 
that  there  has  been  any  shifting  of  the  machinery  employed  for 
carrying  out  the  movement.  So  that  a  volitional  origin  is  not  an 
essential  feature  of  these  so-called  voluntary  movements,  and  the 
machinery  of  the  motor  cortex  and  pyramidal  tract  is  available  for 
other  things  than  pure  volitional  impulses. 

§  663.  The  preceding  discussion  will  enable  us  to  be  very 
brief  concerning  a  question  which  has  from  time  to  time  been 
much  discussed,  and  which  has  acquired  perhaps  factitious  im- 
portance, viz.  the  question  as  to  how  volitional  impulses  leading 
to  voluntary  movements  travel  along  the  spinal  cord.  The  con- 


1064      VOLITIONAL   IMPULSES   IN   THE   CORD.     [BOOK  in. 

elusion  at  which  we  have  arrived,  namely,  that  in  the  normal 
carrying  out  of  voluntary  movements  the  chief  part  is  played  by 
efferent  impulses  passing  along  the  pyramidal  tract.,  carries  with 
it  the  answer  that  volitional  impulses  travel  in  the  spinal  cord 
along  the  pyramidal  tract. 

In  the  dog,  in  which  the  whole  pyramidal  tract  crosses  at  the 
decussation  of  the  pyramids,  we  should  expect  to  find  that  a  break 
in  the  pyramidal  tract  of  one  side  of  the  cord  at  any  point  along 
its  length  caused  loss  of  voluntary  movement  on  the  same  side 
below  the  level  of  the  break.  And  experiments  as  far  as  they  go 
support  this  view.  No  one  it  is  true  has  attempted  to  divide  or 
otherwise  cause  a  break  in  the  pyramidal  tract  alone,  leaving  the 
rest  of  the  cord  intact ;  and  indeed,  even  if  an  injury  were  limited 
to  the  area  marked  out  as  the  pyramidal  tract,  fibres  other  than 
pyramidal  fibres  would  be  injured  at  the  same  time,  since,  the  tract 
is  never  a  '  pure '  one.  But  it  has  been  found  that  a  section  of  a 
lateral  half  of  the  cord,  a  lateral  hemisection,  or  a  section  limited 
to  the  lateral  column  of  one  side  has  for  one  of  its  principal  effects 
loss  of  voluntary  movement  on  the  same  side  in  the  parts  supplied 
by  motor  nerves  leaving  the  cord  below  the  level  of  the  section. 
We  say  '  one  of  its  principal  effects '  because,  besides  the  concomi- 
tant interference  with  sensations  concerning  which  we  shall  speak 
presently,  the  loss  of  voluntary  movement  is  not  absolutely  con- 
fined to  the  same  side ;  there  is  some  loss  of  power  on  the  crossed 
side,  at  least  in  a  large  number  of  cases.  We  must  not  lay  stress 
on  this  crossed  paralysis  because  it  is  probably  one  of  the  effects  of 
the  mere  operation,  not  a  pure  '  deficiency '  phenomenon,  and 
indeed  appears  soon  to  pass  away.  But  taking  into  consideration 
what  was  said  above  concerning  the  effects  of  removing  cortical 
areas,  it  is  important  to  note  that  in  the  experience  of  many 
experimenters  the  loss  of  voluntary  power  on  the  operated  side 
diminishes  after  a  while,  and  that  the  animal  if  kept  alive  and  in 
good  health  long  enough  appears  to  regain  almost  full  voluntary 
power  over  the  affected  parts.  In  such  cases,  as  in  other  operations 
on  the  central  nervous  system,  there  is  no  regeneration  of  nervous 
tissue ;  the  two  surfaces  of  the  section  unite  by  connective  not 
nervous  tissue,  and  the  tracts  which  as  the  result  of  the  section 
degenerate  downwards  or  upwards  are  permanently  lost.  Hence 
even  if  we  admit  that  in  the  intact  animal  a  voluntary  movement 
is  chiefly  carried  out  by  means  of  efferent  impulses  passing  along 
the  pyramidal  tract  right  down  to  the  motor  mechanisms  of  the 
cord  immediately  connected  with  the  motor  nerves,  we  must  also 
admit  that  the  '  will '  under  changed  circumstances  can  find  other 
channels  for  gaining  access  to  the  same  mechanisms. 

It  has  been  further  observed  that  if  in  the  dog  a  hemisection 
be  made  at  one  level,  for  instance  in  the  lower  thoracic  region  of 
the  cord,  and  then,  after  waiting  until  the  voluntary  power  over 
the  hind  limb  of  that  side  has  returned,  a  second  hemisection,  this 


CHAP.  IL]  THE   BRAIN.  1065 

time  on  the  other  side,  be  made  at  a  higher  level,  this  second 
operation  is  followed  by  results  similar  to  those  of  the  first ;  there 
is  loss  of  voluntary  power  on  the  side  operated  on,  with  some  loss 
of  power  on  the  crossed  side,  and  as  in  the  first  case  this  loss  of 
power  not  only  on  the  same  but  also  on  the  crossed  side  may 
eventually  disappear.  This  shews  among  other  things  that  the 
recovery  after  the  first  operation  was  not  due  to  the  remaining 
pyramidal  tract  doing  the  work  of  both.  Further,  the  hemisection 
may  be  repeated  a  third  time,  the  third  hemisection  being  on 
the  same  side  as  the  first,  with  at  least  very  considerable  return 
of  power  over  both  limbs.  That  is  to  say,  under  such  abnormal 
circumstances  voluntary  impulses  may,  so  to  speak,  thread  their 
way  in  a  zigzag  manner  from  side  to  side  along  the  mutilated  cord 
until  they  reach  the  appropriate  spinal  motor  mechanisms.  Such 
an  abnormal  state  of  things  does  not  however  really  militate 
against  the  view  that  under  normal  circumstances  volitional 
impulses  .normally  travel  along  the  pyramidal  tract ;  but  it  does 
shew,  what  indeed  has  already  been  shewn  by  the  phenomena  of 
strychnia  poisoning,  §  586,  that  in  the  central  nervous  system  the 
passage  of  nervous  impulses  (using  those  words  in  the  general 
sense  of  changes  propagated  along  nervous  material)  is  not  rigidly 
and  unalterably  fixed  by  the  anatomical  distribution  of  tracts  of 
fibres;  in  all  such  discussions  as  those  in  which  we  are  engaged 
we  must  bear  in  mind  that  physiological  conditions  as  well  as 
anatomical  continuity  are  potent  in  determining  the  passage  of 
these  impulses. 

§  664.  When  we  reflect  on  the  great  prominence  of  the 
pyramidal  tract  in  the  spinal  cord  of  man  as  compared  with  that 
of  the  dog,  we  may  justly  infer  not  only  that  the  pyramidal  tract 
is  under  normal  circumstances  more  exclusively  the  channel  of 
volitional  impulses  in  man  than  in  such  lower  animals,  but  also, 
bearing  in  mind  the  discussion  in  a  previous  chapter,  §  591, 
concerning  the  activities  of  the  spinal  cord  of  man,  that  the 
potential  alternatives  presented  by  the  spinal  cord  of  the  dog  are 
greatly  reduced  in  that  of  man.  And  such  clinical  histories  of 
disease  or  accidental  injury  in  man  as  we  possess  support  this 
conclusion.  Lesions  confined  to  one  half  of  the  cord,  or  even 
lesions  confined  to  the  lateral  column  of  one  half,  appear  to  lead 
to  loss  of  voluntary  power  on  the  same  side,  and  the  same  side 
only,  in  the  parts  below  the  level  of  the  lesion ;  and  the  same 
symptoms  have  been  observed  to  accompany  disease  limited 
apparently  to  the  pyramidal  tract  of  one  side.  Moreover,  though 
cases  of  recovery  of  power  have  been  recorded,  we  have  not  such 
satisfactory  evidence  as  in  animals  of  the  volitional  impulses 
ultimately  making  their  way  along  an  alternative  route ;  but  here 
the  same  doubts  may  be  entertained  as  were  expressed  in  dis- 
cussing the  reflex  acts  of  the  cord  in  man. 

When  we  say  that  the  loss  of  voluntary  power  is  seen  on  the 


1066  VOLUNTARY   MOVEMENTS.  [BOOK  in. 

side  of  the  lesion  only,  we  should  add  that  this  statement  appears 
to  apply  chiefly  to  the  thoracic  and  lower  parts  of  the  cord.  We 
have  seen  that  in  man,  in  the  upper  regions  of  the  cord,  the 
pyramidal  tract  is  only  partly  crossed;  a  variable  but  not  in- 
considerable number  of  the  pyramidal  fibres  do  not  cross  at  the 
decussation  of  pyramids,  but  running  straight  down  as  the  direct 
pyramidal  tract  effect  their  crossing  lower  down  in  the  cervical 
and  upper  thoracic  regions.  Hence  we  should  infer  that  a  hemi- 
section  of,  or  a  lesion  confined  to  one  side  of  the  cervical  cord, 
would  affect  the  voluntary  movements  of  the  crossed  side  as  well 
as  of  the  same  side,  though  not  to  the  same  extent.  But  we  have 
no  exact  information  as  to  this  point.  And  indeed  the  purpose  of 
the  direct  tract  is  not  clear ;  there  is  no  adequate  evidence  for  the 
view  which  has  been  held  that  these  direct  fibres  are  destined  for 
the  upper  limbs  and  upper  part  of  the  body ;  since  they  are  the 
last  to  cross  we  should  a  priori  be  inclined  to  suppose  that  they 
were  distributed  to  lower  rather  than  higher  parts. 

§  665.  We  may  now  briefly  summarise  what  we  know  con- 
cerning voluntary  movements.  And  it  will  be  convenient  to  trace 
the  events  in  order  backwards. 

Certain  muscles  are  thrown  into  a  contraction  which  even  in 
the  briefest  movements  is  probably  of  the  nature  of  a  tetanus.  In 
almost  every  movement  more  than  one  muscle  as  defined  by  the 
anatomists  is  engaged,  and  in  many  movements  a  part  of  several 
muscles  is  employed,  and  not  the  whole  of  each.  It  is  perhaps 
partly  owing  to  the  latter  fact  that  a  muscle  which  has  become 
tired  in  one  kind  of  movement,  may  shew  little  or  no  fatigue  when 
employed  for  another  movement,  though  we  must  bear  in  mind 
that  in  a  voluntary  movement  fatigue  is  much  more  of. nervous 
than  of  muscular  origin. 

Besides  the  active  muscles,  if  we  may  so  call  them,  which 
directly  carry  out  the  movement,  the  metabolism  of  which  supplies 
the  energy  given  out  as  work  done,  other  muscles,  some  of  which 
are  antagonistic  to  the  active  muscles  and  some  of  which  may  be 
spoken  of  as  adjuvant,  enter  into  the  whole  act.  In  flexion  for 
instance  of  the  forearm  on  the  arm  it  is  not  the  flexor  muscles  only 
but  the  extensors  also  which  are  engaged.  According  to  the 
immediately  preceding  position  and  use  of  the  arm,  and  according 
to  the  kind  and  amount  of  flexion  which  is  to  be  carried  out,  the 
extensors  will  be  either  relaxed,  that  is  to  say  inhibited,  or  thrown 
into  a  certain  amount  of  contraction.  And  in  some  of  the  more 
complicated  voluntary  movements  the  part  played  by  adjuvant 
muscles  is  considerable.  Hence  in  a  voluntary  movement  the  will 
has  to  gain  access  not  only  to  the  active  muscles,  but  also  to  the 
antagonistic  and  adjuvant  muscles;  and  every  voluntary  move- 
ment, even  one  of  the  simplest  kind,  is  a  more  or  less  complex  act. 

The  impulses  which  lead  to  the  contraction  of  the  active 
muscles  reach  the  muscles  along  the  fibres  of  the  anterior  roots, 


CHAP.  IL]  THE   BRAIN.  1067 

(we  may  for  the  sake  of  simplicity  take  spinal  nerves  alone, 
neglecting  the  peculiar  cranial  nerves),  and  such  evidence  as  we 
possess  goes  to  shew  that  the  impulses  governing  the  antagonistic 
and  adjuvant  muscles  travel  by  the  anterior  roots  also;  the 
question  whether  the  inhibition  of  the  antagonistic  muscles  when 
it  takes  place,  is  carried  out  by  inhibitory  impulses  passing  as 
such  along  the  fibres,  or  simply  by  central  inhibition  of  pre- 
viously existing  motor  impulses  need  not  be  considered  now. 
These  anterior  roots  are  connected  as  we  have  seen  with  the  grey 
matter  of  the  cord,  and  in  each  hypothetical  segment  of  the  cord 
we  may  recognize  the  existence  of  an  area  of  grey  matter  which, 
though  we  cannot  define  its  limits,  we  may,  led  by  the  analogy  of 
the  cranial  nerves,  call  the  nucleus  of  the  nerve  belonging  to  the 
segment;  and  we  may  further  recognize  in  such  a  nucleus  what 
we  may  call  its  efferent  and  its  afferent  side. 

Every  voluntary  movement,  even  the  simplest,  is  as  we  have 
repeatedly  insisted  a  coordinated  movement,  and  in  its  coordina- 
tion afferent  impulses  play  an  important  part.  The  study  of  reflex 
actions,  §  589,  has  led  us  to  suppose  that  each  spinal  segment 
presents  a  nervous  mechanism  in  which  a  certain  amount  of  co- 
ordination is  already  present,  in  which  efferent  impulses  are 
adjusted  to  afferent  impulses.  But  the  results  obtained  by 
stimulating  separate  anterior  nerve  roots  shew  that,  in  the  case 
of  most  muscles  at  all  events,  the  especially  active  muscles  of  the 
limbs  for  instance,  each  muscle  is  supplied  by  fibres  coming  from 
more  than  one  nerve  root,  that  is  to  say  the  spinal  nucleus,  or  at  least 
the  spinal  motor  mechanism  for  any  one  muscle,  extends  over  two 
or  three  segments.  Hence  a  fortiori  in  a  voluntary  movement, 
involving  as  this  does  in  most  cases'  more  than  one  muscle,  the 
spinal  mechanism  engaged  in  the  act  spreads  over  at  least  two  or 
three  segments,  thus  allowing  of  increased  coordination.  In  that 
coordination  the  impulses  serving  as  the  foundation  of  muscular 
sense  play  an  important  part,  but  other  afferent  impulses,  such  as 
those  from  the  adjoining  skin,  also  have  their  share  in  the  matter; 
and  it  is  worthy  of  notice  that  not  only  is  the  skin  overlying  a 
muscle  served,  broadly  speaking,  by  nerve  roots  of  the  same 
segment  as  the  muscle  itself,  afferent  in  one  case,  efferent  in  the 
other,  but  in  the  parts  of  the  body  where  coordination  is  especially 
complex,  in  the  fingers  for  instance,  not  only  is  each  muscle 
supplied  from  more  than  one  segment,  but  also  each  piece  of  skin 
is  supplied  in  the  same  way  by  the  posterior  roots  of  more  than 
one  nerve. 

In  the  case  of  the  frog  it  is  clear  that  in  reflex  movements  a 
large  amount  of  coordination  is  carried  out  by  these  various  spinal 
mechanisms ;  and  as  we  have  urged,  we  may  safely  infer  that  in 
the  voluntary  movements  of  the  frog,  the  will  makes  use  of  this 
already  existing  coordination,  whatever  be  the  exact  path  by 
which  in  this  animal  the  will  gains  access  to  the  spinal 


1068  VOLUNTARY   MOVEMENTS.  [BOOK  m. 

mechanisms.  In  the  dog  we  may  conclude  that  in  voluntary 
movements  the  spinal  mechanisms,  with  coordinating  functions, 
are  also  set  in  action,  in  this  case  by  impulses  passing  straight 
from  the  cortex  to  the  mechanisms  by  the  pyramidal  tract, 
though  apparently,  in  the  absence  of  the  pyramidal  tract,  the 
will  can  work  upon  the  mechanisms  by  changes  travelling 
through  other  parts  of  the  cerebrospinal  axis.  And  in  the 
monkey  and  man,  subject  to  the  doubts  already  expressed  as  to 
the  potentialities  of  the  human  spinal  cord,  we  may  probably  also 
infer  that  in  each  voluntary  movement  some,  perhaps  we  may  say 
much,  of  the  coordination  is  carried  out  by  the  spinal  mechanism 
set  into  action  through  impulses  along  the  pyramidal  tract.  We 
may  probably  further  infer  that  a  careful  adjustment  obtains 
between  the  beginnings  of  the  pyramidal  tract  in  the  cortex  and 
its  endings  in  the  cord,  so  that  the  topography  of  'areas'  or  ' foci ' 
in  the  cortex  above  is  an  image  or  projection  of  the  spinal 
mechanisms  below. 

The  complex  character,  on  which  we  insisted  just  now,  of 
almost  every  voluntary  movement  necessitates  that  in  every  such 
movement  a  large  area  of  spinal  mechanism  is  involved.  But  this 
is  not  all.  The  movements  of  any  part,  of  the  legs  for  instance, 
are  not  determined,  nor  is  the  coordination  of  the  movements 
effected,  simply  by  what  is  going  on  in  the  legs  and  the  part  of  the 
spinal  cord  belonging  to  them.  The  discussion  in  a  previous 
section  has  shewn  that  much  of  the  coordination  of  the  body  is 
carried  out  by  the  middle  portions  of  the  brain,  and  on  these  the 
motor  area  must  have  its  hold  as  well  as  on  the  spinal  mechanisms. 

The  details  of  the  nature  of  that  hold  are  at  present  unknown 
to  us ;  but  it  must  be  remembered  that  not  all  the  fibres  passing 
down  from  the  motor  region,  not  all  those  even  proceeding  from 
the  densest  and  most  clearly  defined  motor  areas,  are  pyramidal 
fibres.  With  the  pyramidal  fibres  are  mingled  fibres  having  other 
destinations,  and  some  of  these  probably  pass  to  the  thalamus  and 
so  join  the  great  tegmental  region.  Moreover  the  motor  region 
must  have  close  ties  with  other  regions  of  the  cortex  whence  as  we 
have  seen,  §  632,  fibres  pass  to  the  pons  to  make  connections  with  the 
cerebellum.  On  the  other  hand,  as  we  have  seen,  §  612,  the  cere- 
bellum is  especially  connected  with  what  we  may  fairly  consider 
the  afferent  side  of  the  spinal  cord  and  bulb.  These  facts  must 
merely  be  taken  as  indicating  the  possibilities  by  which  the  motor 
region  is  kept  in  touch  with  the  great  coordinating  mechanism ; 
it  would  be  venturesome  at  present  to  say  much  more. 

In  an  ordinary  voluntary  movement  an  intelligent  consciousness 
is  an  essential  element.  But  many  skilled  movements  initiated 
and  repeated  by  help  of  an  intelligent  conscious  volition  may, 
when  the  nervous  machinery  for  carrying  them  out  has  acquired  a 
certain  facility,  (and  in  all  the  higher  processes  of  the  brain  we  must 
recognize  that,  in  nervous  material  at  all  events,  action  determines 


CHAP,  ii.]  THE   BRAIN.  1069 

structure,  meaning  by  structure  molecular  arrangement  and  dis- 
position) be  carried  out  under  appropriate  circumstances  with  so 
little  intervention  of  distinct  consciousness  that  the  movements 
are  then  often  spoken  of  as  involuntary.  All  the  arguments  which 
go  to  shew  that  the  distinctly  conscious  voluntary  skilled  movement 
is  carried  out  by  help  of  the  appropriate  motor  area,  go  to  shew 
that  the  motor  area  must  play  its  part  in  these  involuntary  skilled 
movements  also.  So  that  distinct  consciousness  is  not  a  necessary 
adjunct  to  the  activity  of  a  motor  area.  And  it  is  worthy  of  notice 
that  some  of  these,  in  their  origin,  purely  voluntary  skilled  move- 
ments, which  by  long-continued  training  have  become  almost  as 
purely  involuntary,  are  hampered  rather  than  assisted  by  being 
"thought  about." 

The  word  'training'  suggests  the  reflection  that  the  physio- 
logical interpretation  of  becoming  easy  by  practice  is  that  new 
paths  are  made,  or  the  material  of  old  paths  made  more  mobile 
by  effort  and  use.  We  have  already  urged,  §  581,  that  the  grey 
matter  of  the  spinal  cord  is  a  network,  in  which  the  passage  of 
impulses  is  determined  by  physiological  conditions  rather  than 
anatomical  continuity,  and  the  same  considerations  may  with  still 
greater  force  be  applied  to  the  brain.  We  must  suppose  that 
training  promotes  the  growth  and  molecular  mobility  of  the 
motor  area  and  of  all  its  connections.  There  are  doubtless 
limits  to  the  changes  which  can  be  effected,  but  within  these 
limits  the  will,  blundering  at  first  in  the  maze  of  the  nervous 
network,  gradually  establishes  easy  paths;  though  even  to  the 
end  it  blunders,  in  trying  to  carry  out  one  movement  it  often 
accomplishes  another. 

Lastly,  without  attempting  to  enter  into  psychological  ques- 
tions, we  may  at  least  say  that  the  birth-place  of  what  we  call  the 
'will,'  is  not  conterminous  with  the  motor  area;  the  will  arises 
from  a  complex  series  of  events,  some  of  which  take  place  in  other 
regions  of  the  cortex,  and  probably  in  other  parts  of  the  brain  as 
well.  With  these  parts  the  motor  area  has  ties  concerned  not  in 
the  carrying  out  of  volition,  but  in  the  generation  of  the  will. 
So  that,  looking  round  on  all  sides,  it  is  obvious,  as  we  have  said, 
that  the  motor  area  is  a  mere  link  in  a  complex  chain.  It  is 
moreover  a  link  of  such  a  kind,  that  while  the  changes  which 
the  breaking  of  it  makes  in  the  daily  life  of  a  lowly  animal,  such 
as  the  dog,  in  whom  the  experience  of  the  individual  adds 
relatively  little  to  the  nervous  and  psychical  storehouse  trans- 
mitted from  his  ancestors,  can  hardly  be  appreciated  by  a 
bystander,  those  which  the  breaking  of  it  makes  in  the  daily  life 
of  a  man,  whose  brain  at  any  moment  is  not  only  a  machine  fitted 
for  present  and  future  work  but  a  closely  packed  record  of  his 
past  life,  are  obvious  not  only  to  the  individual  himself,  but  to 
his  fellows. 


SEC.  8.  ON  THE  DEVELOPMENT  WITHIN  THE  CENTRAL 
NERVOUS  SYSTEM  OF  VISUAL  AND  OF  SOME  OTHER 
SENSATIONS. 

Visual  Sensations. 

§  666.  In  the  chain  of  events  through  which  some  influence 
brought  to  bear  on  the  periphery  of  a  sensory  nerve  gives  rise  to 
a  sensation,  we  are  able,  with  more  or  less  success,  to  distinguish 
between  those  events  which  are  determined  by  the  changes  at 
the  periphery  and  those  which  are  the  expression  of  changes 
induced  in  the  central  nervous  system.  Thus  when  certain  rays  of 
light  proceeding  from  an  object  and  falling  upon  the  eye  give  rise 
to  visual  perception  of  the  object,  two  sets  of  events  happen ;  the 
rays  of  light,  by  help  of  the  mechanisms  of  the  eye,  partly  dioptric, 
partly  nervous,  give  rise  to  certain  changes  in  the  fibres  of  the 
optic  nerve,  which  we  may  call  visual  impulses ;  and  these  visual 
impulses  reaching  the  brain  along  the  optic  nerve  give  rise  to 
visual  sensations  and  so  to  visual  perception  of  the  object.  We 
shall  later  on,  under  the  heading  of  "  the  senses,"  deal  chiefly  with 
the  peripheral  events,  and  have  now  to  consider  some  points  con- 
nected with  the  central  events,  to  learn  what  we  know  concerning 
how  the  various  sensory  impulses  travelling  along  the  several  kinds 
of  sensory  nerves  behave  within  the  central  nervous  system.  In 
doing  so  we  shall  have  from  time  to  time  to  refer  to  peripheral 
events,  but  only  occasionally,  and  never  in  any  great  detail.  It 
will  be  convenient  to  begin  with  the  special  sense  of  sight,  and 
we  must  first  briefly  call  attention  to  a  few  points  which  we  shall 
have  to  study  in  fuller  detail  hereafter. 

The  eye  is  so  constructed  that  images  of  external  objects  are 
brought  to  a  focus  on  the  retina,  the  stimulation  of  which  by 
light  starts  the  visual  impulses  along  the  fibres  of  the  optic  nerve ; 
and  the  distinctness  with  which,  by  means  of  the  visual  sensations 
arising  out  of  these  visual  impulses,  we  perceive  external  objects 
is  dependent  on  the  sharpness  of  the  retinal  images.  The  eye  is 
further  so  constructed  that,  in  any  position  of  the  eye,  the  rays  of 
light  proceeding  from  a  portion  only  of  the  external  world  fall 


CHAP,  ii.]  THE   BRAIN.  1071 

upon  the  retina ;  or  in  other  words  in  any  one  position  of  the  eye 
only  a  portion  of  the  external  world  is  visible  at  the  same  time. 
The  portion  so  seen  is  spoken  of  as  the  visual  field  for  that  position. 

The  image  thrown  on  the  retina  is  an  inverted  one,  so  that  the 
top  of  an  actual  object  is  represented  by  the  lower,  and  the  bottom 
by  the  upper  part  of  the  retinal  image ;  similarly  the  actual  left- 
hand  side  of  the  retinal  image  corresponds  to  the  right-hand  side 
of  the  actual  object,  and  the  right-hand  side  to  the  left-hand  side. 
Hence  the  right-hand  half  of  the  visual  field  corresponds  to  the 
left-hand  side  of  the  retina,  and  the  left-hand  half  to  the  right- 
hand  side. 

The  eye  can  be  moved  in  various  directions,  and  since  in  the 
visual  field  the  portion  of  external  nature  which  can  be  seen  at 
the  same  time  differs  with  each  different  position,  a  large  range  of 
vision  is  thus  secured ;  and  this  can  be  further  increased  by  move- 
ments of  the  head.  Moreover  we  normally  make  use  of  two  eyes, 
our  normal  vision  is  binocular;  and  the  visual  field  of  the  right 
eye  differs  from  that  of  the  left  eye.  There  is  one  striking 
difference  which  must  always  be  borne  in  mind.  A  section 
carried  through  the  eye  in  a  vertical  and  front-to-back  plane, 
through  what  we  shall  learn  to  call  the  optic  axis  (Fig.  133,  ox)  (the 
exact  details  of  the  plane  may  be  left  for  the  present),  will  divide 
the  retina  into  two  lateral  halves,  and  in  each  retina  one  half 
will  be  on  the  nasal  side  next  to  the  nose,  and  the  other  half  will 
be  on  the  malar  or  temporal  side,  next  to  the  cheek  or  temple. 
It  must  be  remembered  that  the  nasal  halves  and  temporal  halves 
of  the  two  retinas  do  not  occupy  corresponding  positions  in  space. 
The  temporal  half  of  the  left  retina  is  on  the  left  side  of  its  own 
eye,  whereas  the  temporal  half  on  the  right  retina  is  not  on  the 
left  but  on  the  right  side  of  its  eye ;  and  so  with  the  nasal  halves. 
Now,  in  the  right  eye,  the  right-hand  side  of  the  visual  field 
corresponds  to  the  nasal  half  of  the  retina,  and  the  left-hand  side 
of  the  visual  field  to  the  temporal  half  of  the  retina,  whereas  in 
the  left  eye  the  right-hand  side  of  the  visual  field  corresponds  to 
the  temporal  half  of  the  retina,  and  the  left-hand  side  to  the  nasal 
half.  This  is  shewn  in  Fig.  133,  where  the  left-hand  visual  field 
and  the  retinal  area  concerned  are  shewn  shaded  in  each  eye. 

When  we  look  at  an  object  with  the  two  eyes,  though  two 
retinal  images  are  produced,  one  in  one  eye  and  one  in  the  other, 
we  perceive  one  object  only,  not  two.  This  is  the  essential  fact  of 
binocular  vision ;  when  certain  parts  of  each  retina  are  stimulated 
at  the  same  time  we  are  conscious  of  one  sensation  only,  not  two ; 
and  the  parts  of  the  two  retinas  which,  stimulated  at  the  same 
time,  give  rise  to  one  sensation  are  spoken  of  as  "  corresponding 
parts."  From  the  structure  and  relations  of  the  two  eyes  it  follows 
that  the  temporal  side  of  the  right  and  the  nasal  side  of  the  left  eye 
are  such  corresponding  parts,  while  the  nasal  side  of  the  right  eye 
corresponds  to  the  temporal  side  of  the  left  eye.  But  the  whole 

F.  68 


1072 


VISUAL   SENSATIONS, 
o 


[BOOK  in. 


1 4th  - 

' In      |&,    )Rflc' 

j/  /'i 
•j/  /  \ 


FIG.  133. 


CHAP,  ii.]  THE   BRAIN.  1073 

FIG.  133.     DIAGRAM  TO  ILLUSTRATE  THE  NERVOUS  APPARATUS  OF  VISION  IN  MAN. 

(Sherrington.) 

L.  the  left  eye,  R.  the  right  eye,  o.x.  the  optic  axis.  F.  the  outline  of  the  face 
between  the  eyes,  Op.T.  the  right  optic  tract  (shaded)  supplying,  through  Op. 
De.  the  optic  decussation,  the  temporal  side  of  the  retina  of  the  right  eye  and 
the  nasal  side  of  the  retina  of  the  left  eye.  L.  F.  L.  and  L.  F.  R.  the  left 
visual  fields  of  the  left  and  right  eye  respectively;  the  two  fields  and  the  parts 
of  the  two  retinas  whose  excitation  produces  vision  over  the  fields  are  shaded, 
the  object  a  in  the  field  of  the  right  side  giving  rise  to  an  image  at  a',  and  a 
on  the  left  side  an  image  at  a'. 

The  right  optic  tract  is  represented  as  ending  in  GL.  the  lateral  corpus  genicu- 
latum,  in  Pv.  the  pulvinar,  and  in  AQ.  the  anterior  corpus  quadrigeminum,  all 
three  stippled;  op.  rad.  the  optic  radiation  from  these  bodies  to  R.  Oc.  the 
right  occipital  lobe,  whose  stippled  cortex  indicates  the  "visual  area."  d.  the 
'direct'  tract  to  the  cortex,  c.  c.  corpus  callosum,  cut  across  at  the  splenium, 
L  v.  d.  descending  horn  of  the  lateral  ventricle. 

The  left  side  has  been  utilized  to  indicate  at  F.  shaded  with  lines,  the  cortical 
motor  area  for  the  eyes;  fin.  c.  indicates  the  path  from  it  to  III.  IV.  VI.  the 
nuclei  of  the  third,  fourth  and  sixth  nerves,  p.  b.  the  posterior  longitudinal 
bundle,  shewn  as  a  broken  line.  NC.  the  nucleus  caudatus,  LN.  the  nucleus 
lenticularis  and  TH.  optic  thalamus  shewn  in  outline,  Cia.  the  front  limb, 
Gig.  the  knee,  and  dp.  the  hind  limb  of  the  internal  capsule.  The  outlines 
of  the  fourth  ventricle  4th  Vn.  and  of  the  posterior  corpora  quadrigemina  are 
shewn  by  dotted  lines,  that  of  the  bulb  is  shewn  by  a  fine  line.  p.  the  pineal 
gland. 

of  each  retiua  is  not  employed  in  binocular  vision.  Owing  to  the 
position  of  the  two  eyes  in  relation  to  the  nose,  it  comes  about  that 
an  object  held  very  much  on  one  side,  to  the  left-hand  side  for 
instance,  while  it  is  capable  of  producing  an  image  on  the  extreme 
nasal  side  of  the  left  eye,  and  can  be  seen  therefore  by  that  eye, 
cannot  produce  an  image  on  the  temporal  side  of  the  right  eye ; 
the  nose  blocks  the  way.  It  is  therefore  not  seen  by  the  right 
eye,  and  the  vision  of  it  is  monocular,  by  the  left  eye  only.  In 
Fig.  133  it  may  be  seen  that  the  left  visual  field  of  the  left  eye 
(L.F.L)  extends  more  to  the  left,  and  is  larger  than  the  left  visual 
field  of  the  right  eye  (L.F.R)  and  that  the  right  retinal  area, 
corresponding  to  the  left  visual  field,  extends  farther  along  the 
nasal  side  of  the  left  side  (a'),  than  it  does  along  the  temporal  side 
of  the  right  eye  (a),  the  difference  being  due  to  the  presence  of 
the  nose  (F).  And  similar  conditions  obtain  with  regard  to  the 
extreme  right-hand  side  of  the  visual  field. 

§  667.  After  these  preliminary  statements,  we  may  now  turn 
to  consider  some  anatomical  facts  concerning  the  ending  of  the 
optic  nerve  in  the  brain. 

The  optic  nerve  of  each  eye  consists  of  nerve  fibres  coming  from 
all  parts  of  the  retina  of  that  eye ;  but  the  two  optic  nerves  meet, 
ventral  to  the  floor  of  the  third  ventricle,  cross  each  other  at 
the  optic  chiasma  (Fig.  133,  op.  De),  and  are  thence  continued  on 
under  the  name  not  of  optic  nerves  but  of  optic  tracts  (Op.T.). 
The  decussation  of  fibres  which  takes  place  in  the  chiasma  has 
peculiar  characters.  At  their  decussation  (we  are  speaking  now  of 
man)  the  fibres  in  the  optic  nerve  belonging  to  the  temporal  half 

68—2 


1074  VISUAL   SENSATIONS.  [BOOK  in. 

of  the  eye  in  which  the  nerve  ends  pass  into  one  optic  tract,  namely, 
the  optic  tract  of  the  same  side,  while  the  fibres  belonging  to  the 
nasal  half  pass  into  another  optic  tract,  namely,  the  optic  tract  of 
the  opposite  side.  Thus  the  fibres  of  the  temporal  half  of  the  right 
eye  and  of  the  nasal  half  of  the  left  eye  pass  into  the  right  optic 
tract,  and  the  fibres  of  the  nasal  half  of  the  right  eye  and  of  the 
temporal  half  of  the  left  eye  pass  into  the  left  optic  tract.  Compare 
Fig.  133,  in  which  the  fibres  forming  the  right  optic  tract  are 
shaded  while  those  forming  the  left  optic  tract  are  left  unshaded. 
Now,  the  nasal  half  of  one  retina  and  the  temporal  half  of  the  other 
retina  are  'corresponding'  parts.  Hence,  while  each  optic  tract 
contains  fibres  belonging  to  half  of  each  eye,  the  two  halves  thus 
represented  in  each  tract  are  corresponding  halves. 

The  amount  and  character  of  the  decussation  taking  place  in 
the  optic  chiasma  differs  in  different  animal  types,  the  differences 
having  relation  to  the  amount  of  binocular  vision,  which  in  turn 
depends  on  the  position  of  the  eyes  in  the  head,  that  is,  on  the 
prominence  of  the  face  between  the  eyes.  In  the  fish  for  instance, 
with  laterally  placed  eyes,  no  binocular  vision  at  all  is  possible, 
and  the  decussation  is  complete ;  the  whole  optic  nerve  of  each  eye 
crosses  over  to  the  other  optic  tract.  Between  this  and  the 
arrangement  in  man  just  described,  various  stages  obtain  in 
various  animals. 

The  chiasma  also  contains  at  its  hinder  part  fibres  which 
have  no  connection  with  the  optic  nerves  or  the  eyes,  but  are 
simply  commissural  tracts  passing  from  one  side  of  the  brain, 
namely,  from  the  median  corpus  geniculatum  (§  630)  along  one 
optic  tract,  through  the  chiasma  to  the  other  optic  tract,  and 
so  to  the  median  corpus  geniculatum  of  the  other  side  of  the 
brain.  These  fibres  are  spoken  of  as  the  inferior  or  posterior 
(optic)  commissure  or  arcuate  commissure,  or  Gudden's  commissure. 
It  was  once  thought  that  in  a  similar  way  fibres  passed  from  one 
retina  along  one  optic  nerve,  through  the  front  part  of  the  chiasma 
to  the  other  optic  nerve,  and  so  to  the  other  retina  forming  an 
anterior  (optic)  commissure ;  but  this  seems  to  be  an  error. 

§  668.  The  optic  vesicle  is  as  we  have  seen  budded  off  from 
the  fore-brain  or  forerunner  of  the  third  ventricle,  and  the  optic 
chiasma  is  attached  to  and  forms  part  of  the  floor  or  ventral  wall 
of  that  ventricle.  In  a  view  of  the  basal  or  ventral  surface  of  the 
brain  the  diverging  optic  tracts  are  seen  to  separate  the  anterior 
perforated  space  and  lamina  cinerea  in  front  from  the  posterior 
perforated  space,  tuber  cinereum  with  the  infundibulum,  and 
corpora  albicantia  behind,  all  these  being  parts  of  the  floor  of  the 
third  ventricle.  From  the  grey  matter  in  this  floor  fibres,  forming 
what  is  sometimes  spoken  of  as  Meynert's  commissure,  belonging 
neither  to  the  optic  nerves  nor  to  the  inferior  commissure,  join 
the  optic  tracts,  eventually  leaving  them  to  pass  to  the  pes. 
Hence  the  whole  of  the  optic  tract  is  by  no  means  derived  from 


CHAP.  IL]  THE   BRAIN.  1075 

the  optic  nerve,  the  fibres  just  mentioned  and  the  inferior  com- 
missure form  parts  of  the  optic  tract  not  connected  with  the 
retina. 

Each  optic  tract  crosses  obliquely,  being  in  crossing  firmly 
attached  to,  the  ventral  surface  of  the  crus  cerebri  of  the  same 
side,  Fig.  108  C,  and  is  soon  lost  to  view,  being  covered  up  by 
the  temporo-sphenoidal  lobe  of  the  hemisphere.  When  this  is 
removed  the  tract  is  seen  to  sweep  dorsally  round  the  crus, 
towards  the  dorsal  aspect,  and  as  we  have  already  (§  630)  said 
to  become  connected  on  the  farther  side  of  the  crus  with  the  two 
corpora  geniculata,  lateral  and  median.  We  may  say  at  once  that 
the  median  corpus  geniculatum  has  no  connection  with  that  part 
of  the  tract  which  is  derived  from  the  optic  nerve,  and  is  not 
concerned  in  vision,  but  is  connected  with  that  part  of  the  tract, 
sometimes  called  the  median  part,  which  goes  to  form  the  inferior 
commissure.  We  may  confine  our  attention  to  that  part  of  the 
tract  which  consists  exclusively  of  fibres  coming  from  the  retinas 
of  the  two  eyes,  for  it  is  this  part,  and  this  part  only,  which  is 
concerned  in  vision. 

§  669.  This  ends  in  three  main  ways,  as  shewn  diagrammati- 
cally  in  Fig.  133.  In  the  first  place  part  of  the  tract  ends  in  the 
lateral  corpus  geniculatum  (GL),  formed  of  alternating  layers  of 
white  and  grey  matter,  the  grey  matter  containing  in  some 
parts  large  nerve  cells,  and  in  others  small  nerve  cells.  In  these 
cells,  of  one  kind  or  another,  many  of  the  fibres  appear  to  end. 
In  the  second  place,  a  very  large  number  of  fibres  passing  the 
corpus  geniculatum  on  its  ventral  and  lateral  surfaces  spread  out 
into  the  pulvinar  (PF).  In  the  third  place  others,  in  considerable 
number,  taking  a  more  median  direction,  reach  the  anterior  corpus 
quadrigeminum  (AQ).  These  two  sets  also,  like  the  first,  end 
apparently  in  the  nerve  cells  of  the  respective  bodies.  Thus  the 
really  optic  fibres  of  the  optic  tract  end  in  one  of  three  collections 
of  grey  matter,  the  lateral  corpus  geniculatum,  the  pulvinar,  and 
the  anterior  corpus  quadrigeminum.  Further,  we  have  reasons 
for  thinking  that  a  considerable  part  at  all  events  of  the  grey 
matter  of  these  three  bodies  is  associated  with  and,  in  a  certain 
sense,  dependent  on  the  fibres  of  the  optic  nerves ;  the  reasons  are 
as  follows.  We  know  that  when  a  nerve  fibre  is  cut  away  from 
its  trophic  centre  it  degenerates ;  but  the  division,  and  the  loss  of 
the  peripheral  degenerating  portion,  has  no  obvious  effect  on  the 
trophic  centre ;  when  a  spinal  nerve,  for  instance,  is  divided  below 
the  spinal  ganglion,  though  the  nerve  below  the  section  degenerates, 
the  ganglion  and  the  piece  of  nerve  in  connection  with  it  remain 
very  much  as  before.  We  have  it,  however,  in  our  power  to 
bring  about  changes  of  a  deeper  and  wider  character,  a  cessation 
of  growth  amounting  to  atrophy,  by  operative  interference  with 
nervous  structures  before  they  are  fully  developed.  Thus  in  an 
adul^  animal,  a  section  of  an  optic  nerve  or  removal  of  the  eye 


1076  VISUAL   SENSATIONS.  [BOOK  m. 

leads  to  degeneration  in  the  optic  nerve  and  optic  tract ;  the 
optic  fibres  have  their  trophic  centre  in  certain  cells  of  the  retina, 
of  which  we  shall  speak  in  treating  of  vision,  and  cut  away 
from  that  centre  they  degenerate ;  by  this  means  the  nature  of 
the  optic  decussation  in  animals,  and  indeed  in  man,  has  been 
ascertained.  But  if  the  eyes  be  removed  (removal  of  both  eyes 
being  desirable  on  account  of  the  characters  of  the  optic 
decussation),  in  a  new-born  animal,  not  only  do  both  the  optic 
nerves  and  the  greater  part  of  both  optic  tracts  cease  to  be 
further  developed  and  degenerate,  but  the  bodies  mentioned 
above,  the  two  lateral  corpora  geniculata,  the  pulvinar  on  each 
side,  and  the  two  anterior  corpora  quadrigemina  do  not  fully 
develope ;  certain  parts  of  them  undergo  atrophy.  The  develop- 
ment of  these  nervous  structures  seems  therefore  to  be  largely 
dependent  on  their  functional  connection  with  the  eyes  by  means 
of  the  optic  tracts  and  nerves. 

The  same  method  confirms  the  view  expressed  above  that  the 
median  corpus  geniculatum  has  no  connection  with  vision.  When 
the  eyes  of  new-born  animals  are  extirpated  neither  the  median 
corpora  geniculata  nor  the  posterior  corpora  quadrigemina  shew 
any  sign  of  atrophy,  and  the  part  of  the  optic  tract  which  does 
not  degenerate  is  the  inferior  commissure  connecting  the  two 
median  corpora  geniculata.  Obviously  these  parts  are  associated 
with  functions  of  the  brain  other  than  those  of  sight.  The  lateral 
corpora  geniculata,  the  pulvinar  and  the  anterior  corpora  quadri- 
gemina, are,  we  may  repeat,  alone  to  be  regarded  as  the  chief 
central  parts  in  which  the  optic  nerves  end.  We  may  also  repeat 
that  owing  to  the  peculiarity  of  the  optic  decussation  each  optic 
nerve  thus  finds  its  endings  in  both  sides  of  the  brain. 

While  the  optic  chiasma  is,  as  we  have  seen,  helping  to  form 
the  floor  of  the  third  ventricle,  it  gives  off  fibres  to  the  posterior 
perforated  spot.  Some  of  these  have  been  supposed  to  pass 
directly  in  the  wall  of  the  ventricle  to  the  nucleus  of  the  third 
(oculo-motor)  nerve,  and  to  serve  as  a  channel  for  afferent  impulses, 
causing  constriction  of  the  pupil ;  but  to  this  we  shall  return  in 
dealing  hereafter  with  the  movements  of  the  pupils. 

§  670.  Though  the  above  three  bodies  are  undoubtedly  the 
chief  endings  of  the  optic  nerve,  three  primary  visual  centres,  if  we 
may  so  call  them,  it  is  also  believed  that  some  fibres  of  the  optic 
tract,  making  connections  with  neither  of  these  three  bodies,  pass 
by  the  crus  cerebri  straight  to  certain  parts  of  the  cerebral  hemi- 
sphere (Fig.  133,  d)\  but  this  fourth  ending  is  by  no  means  so 
clearly  established  as  are  the  other  three. 

And  undoubtedly  the  main  connection  of  the  cerebral  hemi- 
sphere with  the  optic  tract  is  not  a  direct  one,  but  an  indirect 
one,  through  the  three  bodies  in  question.  We  said,  §  633,  that 
fibres  proceeding  from  the  occipital  cortex  and  reaching  the 
thalamus  through  the  hind  limb  of  the  internal  capsule  farmed 


CHAP,  ii.]  THE   BRAIN.  1077 

what  was  called  the  'optic  radiation/  These  fibres  beginning 
(or  ending)  in  the  cortex  of  the  occipital  region,  end  (or  begin), 
(Fig.  133,  op.  rad)  to  a  large  extent,  in  the  pulvinar  and  in  the 
lateral  corpus  geniculatum,  but  also  in  the  anterior  corpus 
quadrigeminum,  reaching  it  by  the  anterior  brachium  (§  634). 
When  even  in  a  grown  animal  the  occipital  cortex  is  destroyed, 
not  only  these  fibres  but  also  parts  of  the  pulvinar  and  external 
corpus  geniculatum  undergo  degeneration,  and  there  is  some 
change  in  the  anterior  corpus  quadrigeminum.  When  the  same 
cortex  is  destroyed  in  a  new-born  animal  the  same  parts  atrophy ; 
and  in  such  cases  the  optic  tract  and  nerve,  which  are  but  little 
affected  by  the  operation  in  the  adult  animal,  are  also  involved  in 
the  atrophy.  We  may  add  that  removal  of  both  eyes  in  the 
new-born  animal  is  said  to  lead,  besides  the  atrophy  of  the  three 
bodies  in  question,  to  a  diminished  occipital  lobe  due  to  lack  of 
white  matter.  We  may  therefore  conclude  that  in  the  complex 
act  of  vision  two  orders  of  central  apparatus  are  involved;  we 
may  speak  of  two  kinds  of  centres  for  vision,  the  primary  or 
lower  visual  centres  supplied  by  the  three  bodies  of  which  we  are 
speaking,  and  a  secondary  or  higher  visual  centre  supplied  by  the 
cortex  in  the  occipital  region  of  the  cerebrum.  And  experimental 
results  accord  with  this  view. 

Before  we  proceed  to  discuss  those  results,  one  or  two  pre- 
liminary observations  may  prove  of  use. 

In  the  first  place,  as  we  have  previously  urged,  the  interpreta- 
tion of  the  results  of  an  experiment  in  which  we  have  to  judge 
of  sensory  effects,  are  far  more  uncertain  than  when  we  have  to 
j  udge  of  motor  effects,  that  is  of  course  when  the  experiment  is 
conducted  on  an  animal.  We  can  estimate  the  motor  effect 
quantitatively,  we  can  measure  and  record  the  contraction  of  the 
muscle ;  but  in  estimating  a  sensory  effect  we  have  to  depend  on 
signs,  our  interpretation  of  which  is  based  on  analogies  which  may 
or  may  not  be  misleading.  We  are  on  safer  ground  when  we  can 
appeal  to  man  himself  in  the  experiments  instituted  by  disease ; 
but  the  many  advantages  thus  secured  are  often  more  than 
counterbalanced  by  the  diffuse  characters,  or  the  complex  con- 
comitants of  the  lesion.  In  dealing  with  sensory  effects  we  must 
expect  and  be  content  for  the  present  with  conclusions  less  defi- 
nite and  more  uncertain  even  than  those  gained  by  the  study  of 
motor  effects. 

In  the  second  place,  in  dealing  with  vision,  it  will  be  desirable 
to  know  the  meaning  which  we  are  attaching  to  the  words  which 
we  employ.  By  blindness,  that  is  'complete'  or  'total'  blindness, 
we  mean  that  the  movements  and  other  actions  of  the  body  are  in 
no  way  at  all  influenced  by  the  amount  of  light  falling  on  the 
retma.  Of  partial  or  incomplete  or  imperfect  vision,  using  the 
word  vision  in  its  widest  sense,  there  are  many  varieties;  and  we 
may  illustrate  some  of  the  defects  of  the  visual  machinery,  re- 


1078  VISUAL   SENSATIONS.  [BOOK  in. 

garded  as  a  whole,  with  its  central  as  well  as  its  peripheral  parts, 
by  referring  to  certain  defects  of  vision  due  to  changes  in  the  eye 
itself.  The  eye  may  fall  into  such  a  condition,  that  the  mind  can 
only  appreciate,  and  that  to  a  varying  degree,  the  difference 
between  light  and  darkness;  the  mind  is  aware  that  the  retina 
(or  it  may  be  part  of  the  retina)  is  being  stimulated  to  a  less 
or  greater  degree,  but  cannot  perceive  that  one  part  of  the  retina 
is  being  stimulated  in  a  different  way  from  another  part ;  a 
sensation  of  light  is  excited,  but  not  a  set  of  visual  sensations 
corresponding  to  the  sets  of  pencils  of  luminous  rays,  which, 
reflected,  or  emanating  from  external  objects  in  a  definite  order, 
are  falling  upon  the  eye.  The  eye  again  may  fall  into  another 
condition,  in  which  such  sets  of  visual  sensations  are  excited,  but 
on  account  of  dioptric  imperfections  or  for  other  reasons,  the 
several  sensations  are  not  adequately  distinct ;  the  mind  is  aware 
through  the  eye  of  the  existence  of '  things,'  but  cannot  adequately 
recognize  the  characters  of  those  things ;  the  visual  images  are 
blurred  and  indistinct.  And  a  large  number  of  gradations  are 
possible  between  the  extreme  condition  in  which  only  those 
objects  which  present  the  strongest  contrast  with  their  surround- 
ings are  visible,  to  a  condition  which  only  just  falls  short  of  normal 
vision.  Imperfections  of  this  kind,  of  varying  degree,  may  result 
from  failure  not  in  the  peripheral  apparatus,  not  in  the  retina,  or 
optic  nerve  or  other  parts  of  the  eye,  but  in  the  central  apparatus ; 
the  retinal  image  may  be  sharp,  the  retina  and  the  optic  fibres 
may  be  duly  responsive,  but  from  something  wrong  in  some  part 
or  other  of  the  brain,  the  visual  sensations  excited  by  the  visual 
impulses  may  fail  in  distinctness,  and  that  in  varying  degree: 
imperfections  of  vision  whether  of  central  or  peripheral  origin,  in 
which  visual  sensations  fail  in  distinctness  are  generally  spoken  of 
under  the  not  wholly  unexceptionable  name  of  amblyopia. 

If  one  optic  nerve  be  divided,  total  blindness  of  one  eye  will 
result ;  but  if  one  optic  tract  be  divided,  it  follows  from  what  has 
been  said  above,  that  half-blindness  in  the  corresponding  halves  of 
both  eyes  will  result.  If,  for  instance,  the  right  optic  tract  (Fig. 
131,  Op.  T.)  be  divided,  the  left  visual  fields  of  both  eyes  will  be 
blotted  out.  The  same  condition  will  be  brought  about  by  failure 
in  the  optic  tract  at  its  central  ending,  provided  of  course  the 
mischief  be  confined  to  the  ending  of  the  one  tract.  Such  a  half- 
blindness  or  half- vision  is  spoken  of  as  hemianopsia,  or  hemianopia 
or  hemiopia ;  the  words  left  and  right  are  generally  used  in 
reference  to  the  visual  field ;  thus  left  hemianopsia  is  the  blotting 
out  of  both  left  visual  fields,  through  failure  of  the  right  optic 
tract. 

If  instead  of  the  whole  optic  nerve  being  divided,  certain 
bundles  only  were  cut  across,  partial  blindness  would  be  "the 
result,  a  portion  of  the  visual  field  would  be  blotted  out ;  and 
mischief  limited  to  a  few  bundles  of  one  optic  tract  would  lead 


CHAP,  ii.]  THE   BRAIN.  1079 

to  corresponding  blots  in  the  corresponding  halves  of  the  visual 
fields  of  both  eyes. 

Further,  an  affection  of  half  the  retina  or  of  a  limited  area  in 
the  retina  might  occur  of  such  a  character  as  to  lead  not  to 
complete,  but  to  partial  blindness,  to  a  hemi-amblyopia  or  to  a 
partial  amblyopia.  The  part  of  the  retina  so  affected  might 
be  central,  or  peripheral,  or  a  quadrant,  or  any  patch  of  any  size, 
form  and  relative  position.  And  we  may  further  imagine  it  at 
least  possible  that  mischief  in  the  brain  might  be  so  limited  as  to 
produce  any  of  the  above  partial  effects,  though  the  retina,  optic 
nerve,  and  optic  tracts  all  remained  intact. 

The  above  visual  imperfections  we  have  illustrated  by  changes 
in  the  peripheral  apparatus,  but  there  is  a  kind  of  imperfection 
which  we  may  still  call  a  visual  imperfection,  though  it  is  of 
purely  central  origin.  In  a  normal  state  of  things  a  visual 
sensation,  excited  in  the  brain,  is  or  may  be  linked  on  to  a  chain 
of  psychical  events ;  we  often  then  speak  of  it  as  a  visual  idea. 
When  we  see  a  dog,  the  visual  sensation,  or  rather  the  group  of 
sensations  making  up  the  visual  perception  of  the  dog,  does  not 
exist  by  itself,  apart  from  all  the  other  events  of  the  brain ;  it 
joins  and  affects  them,  and  among  the  events  which  it  so  affects 
may  be  and  often  are  psychical  events ;  the  visual  perception 
'enters  into  our  thoughts'  and  modifies  them.  Between  the 
visual  impulse  as  it  travels  along  the  optic  nerve  or  tract  and  its 
'ultimate  psychical  effect  a  whole  series  of  events  intervene;  and 
we  may  take  it  for  granted  that  the  chain  may  be  broken  or  spoilt 
at  any  of  its  links,  at  the  later  as  well  as  at  the  earlier  ones. 
We  may  therefore  consider  it  possible  that  the  break  or  damage 
may  occur  at  the  links  by  which  the  fully  developed  visual 
sensation  joins  on  to  psychical  operations.  We  may  suppose  that 
an  object  is  seen  and  yet  does  not  affect  the  mind  at  all  or  affects 
it  in  an  abnormal  way. 

These  foregoing  considerations  emphasize  the  difficulty  and 
uncertainty  of  interpreting  the  visual  condition  of  an  animal 
which  has  been  experimented  upon.  When  for  instance,  after  an 
operation,  an  animal  ceases  to  be  influenced  in  its  previous  normal 
manner  by  the  visual  effects  of  external  objects,  a  most  careful 
psychical  analysis  is  often  necessary  to  enable  us  to  judge  whether 
the  newly  introduced  disregard  of  this  or  that  object  is  due  to  the 
mere  visual  sensations  being  blurred  or  blunted,  or  to  some  failure 
in  the  psychical  appreciation  of  the  sensations ;  and  in  most  cases 
such  an  analysis  is  beyond  our  reach.  The  greatest  caution  is 
needful  in  drawing  conclusions  from  experiments  of  this  kind, 
especially  from  such  as  appear  to  have  been  hastily  carried  out 
or  hastily  observed ;  and  we  must  be  content  here  to  dwell  on 
some  of  the  broader  features  only  of  the  subject. 

§  671.  Since  we  have  in  this  matter  to  trust  so  much  to 
analogies  with  our  own  experience,  we  may  turn  at  once  to  the 


1080  VISUAL   SENSATIONS.  [BOOK  in. 

monkey,  as  being  more  instructive  than  any  of  the  lower  animals. 
We  have  already  said  that  electrical  excitation  of  the  occipital 
cortex  behind  the  motor  region  may  produce  movements,  but  that 
these  movements  are  in  character  different  from  those  caused  by 
stimulation  of  the  motor  region  itself.  In  the  monkey  stimulation 
of  parts  of  the  occipital  region,  the  occipital  lobe  and  the  angular 
gyrus  for  instance,  may  give  rise  to  movements  of  the  eyes,  of  the 
eyelids,  and  of  the  head,  that  is  of  the  neck,  all  the  movements  so 
produced  being  such  as  are  ordinarily  connected  with  vision.  It 
will  not  be  profitable  to  enter  here  into  the  details  concerning 
the  exact  topography  of  the  excitable  parts  or  of  the  special 
characters  of  the  movements  so  called  forth.  But  it  is  important 
to  note  that  these  movements  are  unlike  the  movements  excited 
by  stimulation  of  the  appropriate  motor  area  in  as  much  as 
their  occurrence  is  far  less  certain,  they  need  a  stronger  stimulus 
to  bring  them  out,  when  evoked  they  are  feeble,  being  easily 
antagonized  by  appropriate  stimulation  of  the  motor  area,  and 
they  have  a  much  longer  latent  period.  They  are  not  due  to  any 
indirect  stimulation  of  the  motor  area,  through  "association" 
fibres  connecting  the  spot  stimulated  with  the  motor  area  or 
otherwise,  since  they  persist  after  removal  of  the  motor  area. 
Movements  of  this  kind  may  also  be  witnessed  in  the  dog.  They 
are  obviously  the  result  of  impulses  transmitted  in  some  direct 
manner  from  the  cortex  to  some  parts  below,  and  may  be  taken 
as  an  indication  that  the  parts  of  the  cortex  in  question  are  in 
some  way  connected  with  vision.  The  exact  manner  however 
in  which  they  are  brought  about  is  at  present  obscure.  The 
explanation  of  their  genesis  which  is  frequently  offered,  namely, 
that  the  stimulation  so  affects  the  cortical  grey  matter  as  to  give 
rise  to  visual  sensations,  and  that  the  movements  express  these 
sensations,  does  not  seem  satisfactory.  For,  if  it  be  possible  that  the 
gross  changes  which  the  electric  current  sets  going  in  the  cortical 
grey  matter  can  reproduce  the  psychical  events  which  take  place 
in  that  grey  matter  in  the  normal  action  of  the  brain,  we  should 
expect  stimulation  of  any  and  every  part  of  the  cortex  to  call 
forth  some  movement  or  other,  since  it  cannot  be  doubted  that 
every  part  of  the  cortex  is  in  some  way  or  other  engaged  in 
psychical  operations,  and  that  every  psychical  phase  tends  to 
express  itself  in  movement.  Whereas  outside  the  motor  region, 
with  the  exceptions  we  are  now  discussing,  the  cortex  is,  as  we 
have  seen  "  inexcitable,"  and  even  within  the  motor  region  itself 
the  excitable  substance  is  scattered,  with  increasing  segregation 
as  we  advance  along  the  animal  scale,  among  inexcitable  substance. 
When  we  speak  of  the  region,  or  substance  as  inexcitable,  we  do 
not  mean  that  the  electric  current  produces  no  effect;  we  only 
mean  that  the  effect  is  not  manifested  by  movement ;  the  real 
difference  between  the  excitable  motor  region  and  the  inexcitable 
rest  of  the  cortex  is  probably  that  in  the  several  motor  areas  the 


CHAP,  ii.]  THE   BRAIN.  1081 

current,  playing  upon  the  beginnings  of  the  pyramidal  fibres,  is 
able  to  inaugurate  simple  motor  impulses  or  something  like  them, 
whereas  elsewhere  the  molecular  changes  induced  by  the  current 
are  too  confused  to  reach  their  normal  expression.  There  can 
be  no  doubt  of  course  that  molecular  changes  in  this  or  that 
part  of  the  brain,  set  going  by  processes  other  than  actual  visual 
impulses  along  the  optic  nerves,  may  give  rise  to  visual  sensations ; 
and  as  we  shall  see  in  dealing  with  the  senses  the  subject  of 
such  'subjective'  sensations  is  unable  to  distinguish  them  from 
sensations  of  'objective'  origin;  but  it  is  at  least  unlikely  that 
the  coarse  disturbances  started  by  a  tetanizing  current  should 
take  such  a  definite  form.  Moreover  the  view  in  question  is 
disproved  by  the  experimental  result  that  the  same  movements 
are  brought  about  when  the  cortex  is  pared  away  and  the 
electrodes  are  applied  to  the  subjacent  white  matter.  This 
result  suggests  the  existence  of  efferent  tracts  or  bundles  of  a 
special  kind,  differing  from  those  of  the  pyramidal  kind,  though 
like  them  making  connections  with  the  ocular  and  other  muscles ; 
we  have,  however,  as  yet  no  other  evidence  of  such  tracts  ex- 
isting. 

§  672.  The  results  of  removal  of  the  cortex  also  support  the 
same  general  conclusion,  though  there  is  much  discordance  among 
the  various  observers  both  as  to  the  particular  results  and  es- 
pecially as  to  their  interpretation.  One  broad  fact  comes  out  in 
all  the  observations,  namely,  that  removal  of  or  injury  to  the 
hind  region  of  the  cortex  always  produces  some  disturbance  of 
vision,  and  produces  disturbance  of  vision  more  surely  and  to  a 
greater  extent  than  does  injury  to  or  removal  of  any  other  region 
of  the  cortex;  but  beyond  this  broad  fact  there  is  much  dispute, 
and  we  must  be  content  here  with  a  very  brief  statement. 

In  the  monkey  some  observers  have  found  that  removal  of  the 
occipital  lobe  on  one  side,  the  region  marked  'vision'  in  Figs.  126, 
127,  caused  hemiopia,  the  effect  on  the  visual  fields  being  a  crossed 
one ;  when  the  right  lobe  was  removed  there  was  blindness  in  the 
left  visual  fields,  that  is  in  the  right  halves  of  the  retinas  of  both 
eyes ;  in  other  words  the  visual  impulses  passing  along  the  right 
optic  tract  failed  to  produce  their  usual  effect,  so  that  the  animal 
disregarded  objects  on  its  left-hand  side.  We  may  remark  that 
the  decussation  of  the  optic  nerves  in  the  monkey  is  very  similar 
to  that  in  man.  When  both  occipital  lobes  were  removed,  total 
blindness  resulted.  But,  and  this  is  most  important,  not  only 
was  the  hemiopia,  caused  by  the  removal  of  one  lobe,  transient, 
but  also,  according  to  some  observers,  the  lost  vision  returned 
after  the  total  removal  of  both  lobes,  though  some  impairment 
might  be  noticed  long  afterwards,  so  long  in  fact  as  the  animal 
was  kept  alive. 

In  the  hands  of  other  observers  destruction  of  the  angular 
gyrus  of  one  side  (Fig.  125)  has  led  to  hemiopia,  failure  in  the  left 


1082  VISUAL   SENSATIONS.  [BOOK  m. 

(or  right)  visual  fields,  indicating  failure  in  the  central  endings 
of  the  right  (or  left)  optic  tract,  being  caused  by  removal  of  the 
right  (or  left)  gyrus,  and  destruction  of  both  angular  gyri  has  led 
to  total  blindness,  not  only  the  hemiopia  but  the  total  blindness 
being,  however,  apparently  transitory.  And  cases  have  been 
observed  in  which  the  transient  blindness  due  to  removal  of  the 
occipital  lobes  has  been  succeeded  by  permanent  hemiopia  upon 
the  subsequent  removal  of  the  angular  gyrus.  Indeed  the  general, 
but  not  uniform,  tendency  of  the  many  experiments  which  have 
been  made  is  to  connect,  in  the  monkey,  both  the  occipital  lobe 
and  the 'angular  gyrus  with  vision. 

In  the  dog,  removal  of  portions  of  the  occipital  cortex  have  also 
led  to  partial  and  transient  blindness,  or  according  to  some  to 
permanent  blindness;  but  the  difficulties  of  judging  of  the  visual 
condition  of  a  dog  are  very  considerable,  and  his  vision  is  so 
different  from  that  of  man,  so  much  less  binocular,  for  instance, 
than  his,  that  it  would  not  be  profitable  to  relate  at  length  the 
results  obtained  in  the  dog,  or  to  discuss  the  conclusions  which 
have  been  derived  from  them.  We  will  only  say  that  some 
observers  have  been  led  to  think  that  the  lateral  part  of  the 
retina  is  connected  with  the  lateral  part  of  the  visual  occipital 
area,  the  front  part  with  the  front  part  and  so  on,  the  retina  being 
as  it  were  projected  on  to  the  occipital  cortex ;  but  the  facts  are 
not  clear  enough  to  make  it  worth  while  to  dwell  upon  them  here. 

In  man  clinical  histories  so  far  conform  to  the  results  of 
experiments  on  the  monkey  as  to  associate  the  occipital  cortex, 
and  more  particularly  the  cuneus  (see  Figs.  129,  1.30)  with  vision. 
They,  have  however  raised  a  point  on  which  we  have  not  yet 
touched.  In  the  experiments  on  the  monkey,  quoted  above,  the 
result  (putting  aside  transient  effects  due  probably  to  'shock')  of 
interference  with  one  side  of  the  brain  was  hemiopia;  and  this  is 
what  we  might  expect  from  the  anatomical  relations  ;  the  optic 
tract  goes  straight  to  the  tegmental  masses  of  its  own  side,  and  the 
optic  radiation  passes  from  those  masses  to  the  occipital  cortex  of 
the  same  side;  there  is  no  decussation  save  of  the  fibres  of  the  optic 
nerve,  as  they  pass  into  the  optic  tract  at  the  chiasma.  Clinical 
histories  teach  the  same  lessons  as  these  experiments  on  animals ; 
lesions  limited  to  the  occipital  lobe,  have  for  a  symptom,  hemiopia ; 
and  this  is  said  to  be  especially  the  result  of  mischief  limited  to 
the  apex  of  the  occipital  lobe,  that  is,  to  the  cuneus.  But  experi- 
ments on  monkeys  have  been  made  in  which  destruction  of  one 
angular  gyrus  has  produced,  not  hemiopia,  but  crossed  blindness 
or  crossed  amblyopia,  that  is  to  say  has  affected  the  whole  of  the 
retina  of  one  eye,  and  that  the  crossed  eye,  the  eye  of  the  same 
side  not  being,  or  being  supposed  not  to  be,  at  all  affected ;  similar 
results  have  also  been  stated  to  follow  upon  removal  of  one  occipital 
lobe.  And  a  few  clinical  cases  have  been  recorded  in  which  disease, 
especially  of  the  angular  gyrus,  seemed  to  affect  the  vision  of  the 


CHAP,  ii.]  THE   BRAIN.  1083 

whole  of  the  crossed  eye.  (It  must  be  remembered  that  the 
angular  gyrus  of  man  corresponds  to  a  part  only  of  the  whole 
angular  gyrus  of  the  monkey.  Cf.  Fig.  125  with  Fig.  129.)  Some 
authors  have,  in  accordance  with  this,  put  forward  the  theory 
that  the  occipital  lobe  serves  as  a  cortical  centre  for  the  optic 
tract  of  its  own  side  only,  and  so  for  one  half  of  each  retina,  while 
in  front  of  this  on  the  angular  gyrus  is  a  centre  in  which  both 
optic  tracts  are  represented.  But  the  clinical  histories  bearing 
on  this  point  cannot  be  regarded  as  wholly  satisfactory;  and  with 
reference  to  the  experimental  results  we  may  once  more  insist, 
and  the  warning  applies  perhaps  with  particular  force  to  these 
experiments  on  vision,  on  the  danger  of  confounding  those  imme- 
diate effects  of  operative  interference,  which  are  of  the  nature  of 
'shock'  in  the  wide  sense  of  that  word,  with  those  pure  'deficiency' 
phenomena  which  are  alone  the  outcome  of  the  loss  of  the  part 
removed.  It  is  difficult  to  resist  the  conclusion  that  much  of  the 
transitory  blindness  which  is  observed  in  these  experiments 
belongs  to  the  former  category,  that  the  effect  is  transient  because 
it  is  of  the  nature  of  shock  and  not  because  the  loss  of  faculty  is 
supplied  by  some  other  cortical  area  being  subsequently  substituted 
for  the  one  removed.  In  the  dog,  injury  to  the  frontal  region  of 
the  cortex  unaccompanied  by  any  secondary  mischief  in  the 
occipital  region,  has  led  to  impaired  vision;  and  this  was  probably 
an  instance  of  'shock,'  for  we  have  no  other  reason  to  connect  the 
frontal  region  of  the  cortex  with  vision.  We  must  be  very 
careful  in  drawing  the  conclusion  that,  because  an  operation 
produces  transient  blindness,  the  part  operated  on  has  a  direct 
share  in  vision ;  and  we  may  well  hesitate  to  accept  the  view  that 
the  whole  retina  is  represented  in  the  crossed  hemisphere. 

In  conclusion  we  may  say  that,  when  all  the  many  results 
which  have  been  arrived  at  by  experiment  or  by  clinical  obser- 
vation are  duly  weighed,  it  will  be  felt  that  while  the  evidence 
for  the  occipital  lobe,  especially  the  cuneus,  being  concerned  in  the 
matter  is  convincing,  we  cannot  in  the  present  state  of  our 
knowledge,  dogmatically  exclude  the  angular  gyrus,  and  that 
hence  the  only  clear  and  consistent  statement  which  can  be  made 
with  any  confidence  is  the  broad  and  simple  one  that  the  hind 
region  of  the  cortex  is  in  some  way  intimately  concerned  in 
vision. 

§  673.  Such  an  attitude  becomes  all  the  more  necessary 
when  we  ask  ourselves  the  question  what  is  it  which  actually 
takes  place  in  the  cortex  during  vision?  Are  we  to  conceive  of  it 
as  if  a  visual  impulse  set  going  along  the  fibres  of  the  optic  tract 
underwent  no  essential  change  until  it  reached  the  cortex,  as  if 
it  there  suddenly  developed  into  a  'visual  sensation?'  We  can 
hardly  suppose  this.  Between  the  cortex  and  the  optic  tract,  the 
lower  visual  centres,  the  tegmental  masses,  intervene ;  and  we 
can  hardly  suppose  that  interference  with  these  bodies  produces 


1084  VISUAL   SENSATIONS.  [BOOK  in. 

the  same  effect  on  vision  as  simple  section  of  the  optic  tract. 
We  have  seen  in  a  previous  section  that  the  frog  and  the  bird 
certainly,  and  according  to  some  observers  also  the  rabbit,  are  in 
the  absence  of  the  cerebral  hemispheres  not  totally  blind,  their 
movements  being  guided  by  retinal  impressions;  and  cases  are 
recorded  of  the  dog  being  obviously  still  guided  in  some  measure 
by  retinal  impressions  after  the  occipital  lobes  had  been  wholly  or 
almost  wholly  removed.  And,  though  this  is  a  matter  at  present 
outside  exact  knowledge,  and  though  it  is  perhaps  possible  for 
simple  afferent  impulses  to  determine  even  complex  movements 
without  the  intervention  of  'consciousness,'  we  are  probably  justi- 
fied in  assuming  that  the  simple  visual  impulses,  travelling  along 
the  fibres  of  the  optic  tract,  undergo  important  transformations 
in  the  tegmental  masses,  and  that  the  changes  which  are  propa- 
gated along  the  fibres  of  the  optic  radiation,  constitute  something 
•quite  different  from  the  impulses  along  the  optic  tract  or  nerve. 

Judging  from  the  analogy  of  the  motor  region  we  may  probably 
assume  that  in  vision  the  cortical  events  are  psychical  in  nature, 
and  that  the  function  of  the  optic  radiation  is  to  furnish  what  we 
may  call  crude  visual  sensations  for  further  psychical  elaboration. 

Nor  need  this  view  compel  us  to  suppose  that  injury  to,  or 
removal  of  the  cortex  must  produce  only  psychical  blindness  or 
psychical  impairment  of  vision,  though  this  point  has  probably 
not  been  sufficiently  held  in  view  during  the  various  experiments, 
sufficient  care  not  having  been  taken  to  determine  how  far  the 
blindness  was  purely  psychical.  Bearing  in  mind  the  degeneration 
following  upon  lesions  of  the  occipital  cortex,  and  the  far-reaching 
effects  of  any  operation  on  the  brain,  we  may  suppose  that  injury 
to  the  cortex  affects  the  lower  centres  as  well ;  and  some  of  the 
transient  impairment  of  vision,  on  which  we  have  just  dwelt,  may 
perhaps  be  explained  as  the  effect  of  the  cortical  injury  on  the 
lower  centres. 

Although  the  matter  is  thus  in  many  of  its  details  at  present 
outside  our  exact  knowledge,  we  may  probably  conclude  that  in 
the  complex  act  of  complete  vision,  while  part,  especially  the  more 
psychical  part,  is  carried  out  in  the  cortex,  more  particularly  of 
the  occipital  region,  part  is  accomplished  in  the  lower  centres, 
the  tegmental  masses.  As  to  the  several  functions  of  the  three 
masses,  we  know  almost  absolutely  nothing.  Electric  stimulation, 
and  it  is  said,  mechanical  stimulation  also,  of  the  anterior  corpora 
quadrigemina  in  mammals,  or  the  optic  lobes  in  lower  animals 
calls  forth  movements  of  the  eyes,  and  of  various  parts  of  the 
body;  and  removal  of  them  causes  blindness  and  in  some  cases 
loss  of  coordination  of  movements.  Our  knowledge  on  these 
points  is  not  very  exact ;  but  from  the  above  facts  as  well  as  from 
the  connections  of  the  anterior  corpora  quadrigemina  with  the 
parts  of  the  brain  behind  we  may  possibly  suppose  that  these 
lies  are  more  especially  concerned  with  the  part  visual  impulses 


CHAP.  IL]  THE   BRAIN.  1085 

play  in  determining  the  coordination  of  movements.  We  must 
remember,  however,  that  all  three  masses  are  connected  with  the 
cortex,  and  probably  all  three  play  a  part  in  vision  even  of  the 
highest  psychical  kind. 

Sensations  of  Smell. 

§  674.  The  olfactory  nerve,  which  is  undoubtedly  the  nerve 
of  smell,  stands  like  the  optic  nerve  apart  from  the  rest  of  the 
cranial  nerves ;  and  a  few  words  as  to  its  structure  and  relations 
will  be  necessary. 

Lying  on  the  ventral  surface  of  the  anterior  region  of  each 
hemisphere,  on  each  side  of  the  anterior  fissure,  is  seen  the 
olfactory  bulb,  which  is  prolonged  directly  backwards  as  the 
olfactory  tract,  coming  apparently  to  an  end  where  the  hind 
margin  of  the  frontal  lobe  abuts  on  the  anterior  perforated  space 
in  the  floor  of  the  front  part  of  the  third  ventricle.  The  bundles 
of  fibres  forming  the  olfactory  nerve  proper  spring  from  the  bulb, 
which  is  their  immediate  cerebral  origin,  both  bulb  and  tract 
being  really  parts  of  the  cerebrum.  Just  as  the  fore-brain  buds 
off  on  each  side  the  optic  vesicle  to  form  the  optic  nerve,  so  each 
cerebral  vesicle  buds  off  an  olfactory  vesicle,  the  front  part  of 
which  becomes  the  rounded  bulb  and  the  remainder  the  rounded 
trigonal  tract  or  peduncle  connecting  the  bulb  with  the  hemisphere. 
In  man  the  original  cavity  of  the  vesicle  is  obliterated,  being  filled 
up  with  neuroglial  gelatinous  substance,  but  in  the  lower  animals 
remains  as  a  linear  space,  the  ventricle  of  the  olfactory  tract. 

The  bulb  is  a  specialized  mass  of  grey  matter,  forming  a  sort 
of  cap  to  the  end  of  the  tract,  and  presents  some  analogies  with 
the  cortex  of  the  hemisphere.  Along  the  middle  line  lies  the  core 
of  neuroglial  gelatinous  substance ;  but  the  side  of  the  bulb  dorsal 
to  this  core,  in  contact  with  the  hemisphere,  is  much  less  developed 
than  the  side  lying  ventral  to  the  core,  next  to  the  cribriform 
plate  ;  and  we  may  confine  ourselves  to  the  ventral  portion.  Next 
to.  the  neuroglial  core  lies  a  layer  of  longitudinal  medullated  fibres, 
with  which  are  mingled  some  nerve  cells.  This  layer,  which  forms 
the  beginning  of  the  tract  inside  the  bulb,  is  thinnest  at  the 
rounded  front  extremity  of  the  bulb  and  gradually  thickens 
backward.  Next  to  it  lies  a  'nuclear'  layer,  composed  of  small 
nuclear  cells,  arranged  to  a  large  extent  in  longitudinally  disposed 
rows.  Fibres  from  the  preceding  layer  pass  between  the  groups, 
which  are  moreover  separated  by  interlacing  bundles  of  fibres. 
Next  to  this  layer  comes  a  somewhat  thick  one,  which  perhaps 
may  be  compared  to  the  molecular  layer  of  the  cerebellum  or  to 
the  pyramidal  layers  of  the  cerebrum.  It  is  composed  of  a 
molecular  ground  substance,  partly  neuroglial  in  nature,  traversed 
by  numerous  fibrils  and  fibres,  many  of  the  latter  being  of  the 


1086  OLFACTORY   SENSATIONS.  [BOOK  in. 

fine  medullated  kind;  it  also  contains,  in  no  large  number  in 
man,  nerve  cells,  some  of  which  from  their  triangular  form  and 
tapering  branched  processes  are  not  unlike  the  pyramidal  cells  of 
the  cortex.  The  larger  of  these  cells  are  generally  found  near 
the  nuclear  layer.  Next  to  this  molecular  layer,  or  'gelatinous 
layer'  as  it  is  sometimes  called,  comes,  still  working  outwards 
towards  the  surface,  a  characteristic  layer  in  which  are  found  the 
'olfactory  glomeruli';  and  outside  this  is  the  layer  of  olfactory 
fibres  proper,  that  is  to  say,  fibres  non- medullated  (§70)  but 
bearing  an  obvious  neurilemrna.  These  olfactory  fibres  are 
arranged  in  a  close  set  plexus,  and  bundles  of  fibres  gathered 
up  from  the  plexus  at  intervals  pierce  the  pia  mater,  which 
invests  the  bulb  and  furnishes  it  with  an  ample  supply  of  blood 
vessels,  to  form  the  olfactory  nerve  proper.  The  structure  of  the 
olfactory  glomeruli,  which  are  about  '05  mm.  in  diameter,  has 
not  yet  been  fully  made  out;  they  are  described  as  being  formed 
by  coils  of  the  olfactory  fibres  with  small  cells  and  blood  vessels 
interspersed  among  the  coils ;  in  the  lower  animals  a  finely  granu- 
lar ground  substance  is  present.  Fibres  from  the  layers  beneath 
have  been  traced  to  them.  We  may  perhaps  assume  that  they 
serve  as  the  immediate  origin  of  the  olfactory  fibres;  but  their 
exact  relations  to  the  other  layers  of  the  bulb  are  by  no  means 
clear. 

The  tract  is  composed  partly  of  longitudinal  fibres,  with  which 
are  mingled  nerve  cells,  and  partly  of  neuroglial  gelatinous  substance. 
The  fibres  begin  in  the  bulb,  which  appears  to  serve  as  a  relay 
between  them  and  the  fibres  of  the  olfactory  nerve  proper;  and 
while  some  appear  to  end  in  cells  in  the  tract  itself,  others  are 
continued  on  to  the  end  of  the  tract,  being  joined  by  fibres  taking 
origin  along  the  tract.  We  may  compare  the  bulb  and  the  tract 
to  a  part  of  the  retina  (as  we  shall  see,  a  part  of  the  retina 
corresponds  to  the  olfactory  mucous  membrane)  and  the  optic 
nerve. 

The  dorsal  surface  of  the  tract  is  adherent  to  and  continuous 
with  the  substance  of  the  cerebral  hemisphere,  in  a  groove  of 
which  it  lies,  but  the  tract  may  be  considered  as  independent 
of  the  hemisphere  until  it  reaches  its  end,  at  which  it  breaks 
up  into  bands  of  fibres,  spoken  of  as  its  'roots.'  The  most 
conspicuous  of  these  is  a  lateral  one,  which  sweeping  laterally 
across  the  anterior  perforated  space,  at  the  mouth  of  the  fissure  of 
Sylvius,  may  be  traced  to  the  nucleus  amygdalae  (Fig.  116,  Na), 
and  the  junction  of  this  with  the  hippocampal  or  uncinate  gyrus 
(Fig.  130)  in  the  temporal  lobe  of  the  hemisphere  of  the  same 
side.  A  much  smaller  median  one,  which  however  in  some  of 
the  lower  animals  is  large  and  conspicuous,  takes  a  median 
direction,  passes  into  the  anterior  commissure  (§635)  and  so 
reaches  the  olfactory  tract  of  the  opposite  side.  Other  small 
roots  have  also  been  described. 


CHAP.  IL]  THE   BRAIN.  1087 

§  675.  In  many  animals  in  whom  the  sense  of  smell  is  acute, 
a  portion  of  the  cortex,  known  as  the  "  pyriform  lobe  "  or  "  hippo- 
campal  lobule,"  and  which  is  anatomically  continuous  with  the 
front  end  of  the  hippocampal  gyrus  (the  part  to  which  the  name 
uncinate  gyrus  is  often  restricted),  acquires  relatively  large 
dimensions.  This  and  the  anatomical  relations  just  mentioned 
would  lead  us  to  suppose  that  a  part  of  the  cortex  which  is 
continuous  with  the  front  end  of  the  hippocampal  gyrus  is  in 
some  way  connected  with  smell.  The  argument  from  compara- 
tive anatomy,  however,  is  one  which  must  be  used  with  caution ; 
since,  besides  the  great  difficulty  of  determining  the  homologies 
of  parts  of  the  brain  in  different  animals,  relative  increase  in  the 
part  in  question  might  be  correlated  to  other  things  than  the 
power  of  smell,  and  might  be  determined  by  circumstances  having 
no  relation  to  smell. 

The  experimental  evidence,  though  on  the  whole  it  gives 
support  to  the  view,  is  conflicting;  and  when  the  difficulty  of 
determining  whether  a  "dumb  animal"  can  or  cannot  smell  is 
borne  in  mind,  this  will  not  be  wondered  at.  The  observation 
that  electrical  stimulation  of  the  region  in  question  gives  rise  to 
movements  of  the  nostrils,  which  have  been  interpreted  as  sniffing 
in  response  to  subjective  olfactory  sensations,  cannot  have  'much 
weight ;  and  while  some  observers  have  found  that  the  removal 
of  this  part  of  the  brain  destroys  the  sense  of  smell,  others  have 
obtained  negative  results. 

The  few  clinical  histories  which  bear  upon  the  matter  are 
perhaps  more  trustworthy.  These  seem  to  shew  that  a  lesion 
involving  the  cortex  of  this  region,  but  leaving  the  olfactory  bulb 
and  tract,  as  well  as  other  parts  of  the  brain,  intact,  may  destroy 
or  greatly  impair  smell.  And  we  may  perhaps  give  particular 
weight  to  the  cases  in  which  epileptiform  attacks,  preceded  by  an 
'aura'  in  the  form  of  a  peculiar  smell,  have  been  associated  with 
disease  limited  to  this  region ;  for  the  phenomena  of  '  aura '  seem 
to  be  connected  with  cortical  processes. 

Though  the  evidence  on  the  whole  goes  to  shew  that  the 
cortex  at  the  front  end  of  the  hippocampal  gyrus  is  especially 
connected  with  smell,  and  we  have  so  marked  it  in  Fig.  132,  yet 
the  whole  matter  stands  on  a  somewhat  different  footing  from 
the  sense  of  sight.  In  man  the  relations  of  smell  to  the  other 
operations  of  the  brain  (though,  as  we  shall  see  in  dealing  with 
the  senses,  somewhat  peculiar)  are  far  more  limited  than  are 
those  of  vision,  and  the  psychical  development  of  simple  olfactory 
sensations  is  extremely  scanty. 

Sensations  of  Taste. 

§  676.  This  special  sense  though  so  closely  associated  with 
smell  stands,  together  with  the  special  sense  of  hearing,  on  a 

F.  69 


1088  SENSATIONS   OF   HEARING.  [BOOK  in. 

different  footing  from  the  two  preceding  special  senses,  since  the 
nerves  concerned  belong  to  the  category  of  ordinary  cranial 
nerves,  and  we  lack,  in  reference  to  them,  the  anatomical  leading 
which  is  offered  to  us  in  the  case  of  the  optic  and  olfactory  nerves. 
We  shall  see  in  dealing  with  the  senses  that  the  fifth  nerve 
and  the  glossopharyngeal  nerve  have  been  considered  as  nerves 
of  taste,  but  that  the  matter  is  one  subject  to  controversy ;  the 
gustatory  function  of  the  fifth  is  attributed  to  the  peculiar 
chorda  tympani  nerve,  and  other  questions  have  been  raised. 
Whatever  view  we  take,  however,  the  nerves  of  taste  are  ordinary 
cranial  nerves,  and  we  have  no  anatomical  guidance  as  to  the 
fibres  of  either  of  the  above  two  nerves  making  special  connec- 
tions with  any  part  of  the  cortex.  Though  sensations  of  taste 
enter  largely  into  the  life  of  animals,  and  indeed  of  man  himself, 
we  have  no  satisfactory  indications  which  will  enable  us  to 
connect  this  special  sense  with  any  part  of  the  cortex ;  the 
view  indeed  has  been  put  forward  that  some  part  of  the  cortex 
in  the  lower  portion  of  the  temporal  lobe,  not  far  from  the  centre 
for  smell,  serves  as  a  centre  for  taste;  but  the  arguments  in 
favour  of  this  view  are  not,  as  yet  at  least,  convincing. 


Sensations  of  Hearing. 

§  677.  The  cochlear  division  of  the  eighth  or  auditory  nerve 
may  be  assumed  to  be  a  nerve  of  the  special  sense  of  hearing,  and 
of  that  alone ;  the  vestibular  division  serves,  as  we  have  seen,  for 
other  functions  than  those  of  hearing,  §  642,  but  as  we  shall  urge  in 
dealing  with  the  senses  is  not  to  be  regarded  as  wholly  useless  for 
the  purposes  of  that  sense.  The  cochlear  division  we  have  traced, 
§  618,  into  the  bulb,  and  the  vestibular  division  into  the  lateral 
auditory  nucleus  (which  perhaps  may  be  regarded  as  a  continua- 
tion or  segmental  repetition  forwards  of  the  cuneate  nucleus  or  of 
part  of  that  nucleus),  and  into  the  cerebellum,  the  cerebellar 
continuation  being  probably  the  part  of  the  nerve  which  serves  for 
coordinating  functions.  The  connections  of  the  auditory  nerve 
with  the  cerebral  hemisphere  belong  to  the  same  category  as 
those  of  other  afferent  cranial,  and  we  may  add  spinal  nerves; 
we  have  no  very  clear  anatomical  guide  towards  any  particular 
part  of  the  cortex. 

When  we  turn  to  the  empirical  results  furnished  by  experi- 
ment and  clinical  observations,  we  find  that  these,  though  even 
less  definite  and  less  accordant  than  in  the  case  of  the  senses 
of  sight  and  smell,  point  to  part  of  the  first  or  superior  temporal 
(temporo-sphenoidal)  convolution  (Figs.  126,  129,  131)  lying  in 
the  temporal  lobe  just  ventral  to  the  Sylvian  fissure,  as  being 
specially  concerned  in  hearing  in  some  such  way  as  the  occipital 
lobe  is  concerned  in  vision. 


CHAP,  ii.]  THE    BRAIN.  1089 

Electrical  stimulation  of  this  region  of  the  cortex  gives  rise 
to  "pricking  of  the  ears,''  and  other  movements  such  as  are 
frequently  connected  with  auditory  sensations;  but  such  pheno- 
mena are  in  this  instance  perhaps  to  be  depended  upon  even 
less  than  in  other  similar  instances.  While  some  observers 
maintain  that  this  convolution,  the  operation  including  other 
portions  of  the  temporal  lobe  as  well,  may  be  removed  from  a 
monkey  without  producing  any  certain  signs  of  deafness,  other 
observers  have  found  that  removal  of  it  on  one  side  affected 
the  hearing  of  the  ear  on  the  opposite  side,  and  removal  on 
both  sides  brought  the  animal  into  a  condition  in  which,  without 
being  perhaps  absolutely  deaf,  it  reacted  towards  sound  in  a  very 
imperfect  manner  indeed,  very  different  from  its  normal  behaviour. 
The  scanty  clinical  histories  bearing  on  this  matter  are  not  very 
decisive ;  for  though  deafness  has  been  observed  in  connection 
with  disease  affecting  the  superior  temporal  convolution,  the 
lesion  has  usually  invaded  other  parts  as  well,  and  the  deafness 
has  been  associated  with  other  symptoms,  notably  aphasia.  An 
auditory  '  aura '  has  however  at  times  been  observed  in  connection 
with  disease  of  this  region,  as  also  a  peculiar  psychical  failure, 
known  as  "  word  deafness,"  in  which,  though  sounds  are  heard, 
that  is  to  say  auditory  sensations  are  felt,  it  may  be  even  as 
usual,  the  perception  or  psychical  appreciation  of  the  sounds  is 
lacking,  and  a  spoken  word  is  not  recognized. 

Lastly,  we  may  add  that,  though  as  we  said  the  anatomical 
leading  is  not  definite,  observers  have  found  that,  in  new-born 
animals,  on  the  one  hand  destruction  of  the  part  of  the  cortex 
probably  corresponding  to  the  region  mentioned  above,  leads  to 
atrophy  of  the  median  corpus  geniculatum,  and,  to  some  extent, 
of  the  posterior  corpus  quadrigeminum ;  and  on  the  other  hand 
destruction  of  the  internal  ear  leads  to  an  atrophy  of  part  of 
the  lateral  fillet  of  the  opposite  crossed  side  which  may  be  traced 
to  the  posterior  corpus  quadrigeminum,  and  thence  to  the  median 
corpus  geniculatum ;  and  section  of  the  lateral  fillet  on  one  side 
leads,  among  other  results,  to  atrophy  of  the  striae  acusticae 
and  tuberculum  acusticum  (§  618)  of  the  crossed  side.  This 
suggests  that  the  path  of  auditory  impulses  is  along  the  cochlear 
nerve  to  the  lateral  fillet  of  the  crossed  side,  and  so  by  the 
posterior  corpus  quadrigeminum  and  median  corpus  geniculatum 
to  the  cortex  of  the  temporal  lobe  of  that  crossed  side,  the  two 
later  bodies  bearing  towards  hearing  a  relation  somewhat  like 
that  borne  towards  sight  by  the  anterior  corpus  quadrigeminum 
and  lateral  corpus  geniculatum.  But  the  matter  needs  farther 
investigation. 

There  remains  the  special  sense  of  touch,  but  this  we  had 
better  consider  in  connection  with  sensations  in  general. 


69—: 


SEC.    9.     ON   THE   DEVELOPMENT   OF   CUTANEOUS   AND 
SOME   OTHER   SENSATIONS. 


§  678.  The  sensations  with  which  we  have  just  dealt  arise 
through  impulses  passing  along  special  nerves  or  parts  of  special 
nerves,  the  optic  nerve,  the  olfactory  nerve  &c. ;  we  have  now  to 
deal  with  sensations  arising  through  impulses  along  the  nerves 
of  the  body  generally.  These  are  of  several  kinds.  In  the  first 
place  there  are  sensations  which  we  may  speak  of  as  "  cutaneous 
sensations,"  the  impulses  giving  rise  to  which  are  started  in  the 
skin  covering  the  body,  or  in  the  so-called  mucous  membrane 
lining  certain  passages.  These  sensations,  which  as  we  shall  see  in 
dealing  with  the  senses  are  dependent  on  the  existence  of  special 
terminal  organs  in  or  near  the  skin,  are  sensations  of  "  touch," 
in  the  narrower  meaning  of  that  word,  by  which  we  appreciate 
contact  with  and  pressure  on  the  skin,  and  the  sensations  of 
"  temperature,"  which  again  we  may,  as  we  shall  see,  divide  into 
sensations  of  "  heat "  and  sensations  of  "  cold."  These  sensations 
may  be  excited  in  varying  degree  by  impulses  passing  along  any 
nerve  branches  of  which  are  supplied  to  the  skin.  Then  there 
are  the  sensations  constituting  the  "muscular  sense,"  to  which 
we  have  already  referred,  and  these  again  may  be  excited  in  any 
nerve  having  connections  with  the  skeletal  muscles. 

As  we  shall  see  in  dealing  with  the  senses,  when  a  nerve  is  laid 
bare  and  its  fibres  are  stimulated  directly  either  by  pressure, 
such  as  pinching,  or  by  heat,  or  by  cold,  or  in  other  ways,  the 
sensations  which  are  caused  do  not  enable  us  to  appreciate 
whether  the  stimulation  is  one  of  contact  or  pressure,  or  of 
temperature,  or  of  some  other  kind;  we  only  experience  a 
"  feeling,"  which  at  all  events  when  it  reaches  a  certain  intensity 
we  speak  of  as  "pain."  And  we  have  reason  to  think  that  at 
least  from  time  to  time  impulses  along  various  nerves  give  rise  to 
sensations  which  have  been  spoken  of  as  those  of  "general 
sensibility,"  by  which  in  addition  to  other  sensations,  such  as 
those  of  touch  and  of  the  muscular  sense,  we  become  aware  of 
changes  in  the  condition  and  circumstances  of  our  body.  When 
the  stimulation  of  the  skin  exceeds  a  certain  limit  of  intensity, 
the  sense  of  touch  or  temperature  is  lost  in,  that  is  to  say,  is  not 
appreciated  as  separate  from  the  sense  of  pain;  and  under 


CHAP.  IL]  THE   BRAIN.  1091 

abnormal  circumstances  acute  sensations  of  pain  are  started  by 
changes  in  parts,  for  example  tendons,  the  condition  of  which 
under  normal  circumstances  we  are  not  conscious  of  appreciating 
through  any  distinct  sensations,  though  it  may  be  that  these  parts 
do  normally  give  rise  to  feeble  impulses  contributing  to  '  general 
sensibility.'  It  may  therefore  be  debated  whether  'pain'  is  a 
phase  of  all  sensations,  or  of  general  sensibility  alone,  or  a 
sensation  sui  generis.  We  shall  have  something  further  to  say  on 
this  matter  when  we  treat  of  the  senses ;  meanwhile  it  will  be 
convenient  for  present  purposes  if  we  consider  that  the  sensations 
we  have  to  deal  with  just  now  are  the  sensations  of  touch  and 
of  temperature,  those  of  the  muscular  sense,  and  those  of  general 
sensibility  including  those  of  pain. 

§  679.  The  fairly  convincing  evidence  that  the  occipital 
cortex  has  special  relations  with  vision,  and  the  less  clear  evidence 
that  other  regions  have  special  relations  with  smell  and  hearing, 
suggest  that  special  parts  of  the  cortex  have  special  relations  with 
the  sensations  now  under  consideration.  But  in  the  cases  of  the 
senses  of  sight  and  smell  we  had  a  distinct  anatomical  leading ; 
and  we  have  seen  how  uncertain  is  the  evidence  where  such  an 
anatomical  leading  fails,  as  in  hearing  and  taste.  In  the  case  of 
sensations  of  the  body  at  large,  the  anatomical  leading  similarly 
fails.  Moreover  any  attempt  to  push  the  analogy  of  sight  raises 
the  following  question.  If  there  were  two  optic  nerves  on  each 
side  of  the  head,  would  there  be  two  cortical  areas,  one  for  each 
nerve,  in  each  hemisphere,  or  one  visual  area  only  ?  And  again, 
if  the  optic  nerve  were  the  instrument  for  some  sense  in  addition 
to  that  of  sight,  would  there  be  two  cortical  areas,  one  for  each 
sensation,  or  one  area  only  serving  as  the  cortical  station  so  to 
speak  of  the  whole  nerve  ?  If  we  push  the  analogy  of  sight  it 
is  open  for  us,  since  we  cannot  give  a  definite  answer  to  the  above 
question,  to  suppose  either  that  there  is  one  area  for  touch, 
another  area  for  temperature,  and  so  on,  each  for  the  whole  body, 
or  that  there  is  an  area  for  sensations  of  all  kinds  for  each  afferent 
nerve,  or,  that  there  is  an  intricate  arrangement  which  supplies 
all  the  combinations  of  the  two  which  are  required  for  the  life  of 
the  individual.  Of  the  three  hypotheses  the  latter  is  the  more 
probable ;  but  if  so,  it  is  by  its  very  nature  almost  insusceptible 
of  experimental  proof,  especially  when  we  bear  in  mind  what  we 
have  already  said  touching  the  difficulty  of  judging  the  sensa- 
tions of  animals.  If  the  judgment  of  visual  sensations  is  difficult, 
how  much  more  difficult  must  be  the  judgment  of  sensations  of 
touch  and  temperature  ?  Indeed,  sensations  of  pain  are  the  only 
sensations  of  which  we  can  form  a  quantitative  judgment  in 
animals;  and  our  method  of  judging  even  these,  namely,  by 
studying  the  movements  or  other  effects  indirectly  produced, 
is  a  most  imperfect  one. 

We   can   learn   therefore   almost   absolutely  nothing   in    this 


1092  CUTANEOUS   SENSATIONS.  [BOOK  HI. 

matter  from  experimental  stimulation  of  the  cortex  in  animals. 
As  we  have  previously  (§  671)  urged,  the  absence  of  movements 
when  parts  of  the  cortex  other  than  the  motor  region  are  stimu- 
lated is  no  evidence  that  the  stimulation  does  not  give  rise  to 
psychical  events  into  which  sensations  enter ;  and  movements 
follow  stimulation  of  the  motor  area,  not  because  that  area  is 
wholly  given  up  to  motor  events,  but  because  from  the  histological 
arrangement  the  stimulus  gets  ready  access  to  relatively  simple 
motor  mechanisms.  That  the  motor  region  has  close  connections 
with  sensory  factors  is  not  only  almost  certain  on  theoretical 
grounds,  but  is  shewn  in  many  ways,  for  example  by  the 
experiment,  described  in  §  661,  of  exalting  the  sensitiveness  of 
a  motor  area  by  generating  peripheral  sensory  impulses. 

Nor  can  the  effects  on  sensation  of  removal  of  parts  of  the 
cortex  be  interpreted  with  clearness  and  certainty.  In  the  monkey 
removal  or  destruction  of  the  gyrus  fornicatus  (Figs.  125,  127)  on 
the  mesial  surface  of  the  brain,  ventral  to  the  calloso-marginal 
sulcus  which  forms  on  the  mesial  surface  the  ventral  limit  of  the 
motor  region  (an  operation  of  very  great  difficulty),  has  brought 
the  whole  of  the  opposite  side  of  the  body  to  a  condition  which 
has  been  described  as  an  anesthesia,  that  is  a  loss  of  all  cutaneous 
tactile  sensations,  and  an  analgesia,  that  is  a  loss  of  sensations  of 
pain,  the  condition  being  accompanied  by  little  or  no  impairment 
of  voluntary  movements  and,  though  apparently  diminishing  as 
time  went  on,  lasting  until  the  death  of  the  animal  some  weeks 
afterwards.  Again,  removal  of  the  continuation  of  the  gyrus 
fornicatus  into  the  gyrus  hippocampi  has  in  other  instances  led 
to  a  more  transient  anesthesia  also  of  the  whole  or  greater  part 
of  one  side  of  the  body.  And  it  is  asserted  that  removal  of  no 
other  region  of  the  cortex  interferes  with  cutaneous  and  painful 
sensations  in  so  striking  and  lasting  a  manner  as  does  the  removal 
of  parts,  or  of  the  whole  of  this  mesial  region. 

These  results,  however,  do  not  accord  with  clinical  experience, 
which,  though  scanty,  seems  as  far  as  it  goes  to  shew  that  in  man, 
when  mischief  apparently  limited  to  the  cortex  produces  loss  of 
sensations,  it  is  the  parietal  lobe  corresponding  to  the  motor  region 
which  is  affected ;  but  there  appears  to  be  no  record  of  any  case  of 
a  cortical  lesion  affecting  sensation  without  affecting  movement. 
We  have  previously  called  attention  to  the  fact  that  the  temporary 
loss  or  impairment  of  movement  which  follows  removal  of  an  area 
is  frequently,  if  not  always,  accompanied  by  an  impairment  of 
cutaneous  sensations  in  the  limb  or  part  'paralysed;'  and  side  by 
side  with  this  we  may  put  the  experience  that  in  the  human 
epileptiform  attacks  of  cortical  origin,  the  seizure  is  at  times 
ushered  in  by  peculiar  sensations,  called  the  '  aura,'  in  the  part 
movements  of  which  inaugurate  the  march  of  convulsive  move- 
ments. But  these  things  do  not  shew  that  the  cortical  area  is  the 
"  seat  of  sensations,"  they  rather  illustrate  what  we  said  concerning 


CHAP.  IL]  THE   BRAIN.  1093 

the  complexity  of  the  chain  of  which  the  events  in  the  cortical 
area  are  links,  and  the  close  tie  between  sensory  factors  and  the 
characteristic  elements  of  the  motor  region. 

In  the  dog,  while  removal  of  almost  any  considerable  portion 
of  the  cortex  affects  sensation,  removal  of  parts  in  the  frontal 
region  producing  perhaps  less  effect  than  removal  of  parts  in 
other  regions,  the  loss  or  impairment  of  sensation  appears  to  be 
transient,  though  having  a  duration  broadly  proportionate  to  the 
extent  of  cortex  removed ;  and  when  a  very  large  portion  of  the 
cortex  is  removed,  some  imperfection  appears  to  remain  to  the  end. 
We  have  already  referred  to  the  case  of  a  dog  from  which  the 
greater  part  of  both  cerebral  hemispheres  had  been  removed,  but 
which  remained  capable  of  carrying  out  most  of  the  ordinary 
bodily  movements,  and  that  apparently  in  a  voluntary  manner ;  in 
this  case  the  "  blunting"  of  cutaneous  sensations  was  perhaps 
more  striking  than  the  imperfection  of  movement.  It  will  be 
worth  while  to  consider  the  condition  of  this  dog  a  little  closely, 
on  account  of  the  light  which  it  throws  on  the  problem  which  we 
are  now  discussing. 

Clinical  experience  shews  that  in  man  the  integrity  of  the 
cerebral  hemispheres,  and  of  the  connection  of  the  hemispheres 
with  the  rest  of  the  central  nervous  system,  is  essential  to  the 
full  development  of  sensations ;  and  that  in  this  respect  each 
hemisphere  is  related  to  the  crossed  side  of  the  body.  A  very 
common  form  of  paralysis  or  "stroke"  is  that  due  to  a  lesion  of" 
some  part  of  one  hemisphere  (the  exact  position  of  the  lesion  need 
not  concern  us  now),  frequently  caused  by  rupture  of  a  blood  vessel, 
in  which  the  patient  loses  all  power  of  voluntary  movement  and 
all  sensations  on  the  crossed  side  of  his  body  (including  the  face) ; 
he  is  said  to  be  suffering  from  hemiplegia,  "  one  sided  stroke." 
Not  only  do  voluntary  impulses  fail  to  reach  the  muscles  of  the 
affected  side,  but  sensory  impulses,  such  as  those  which,  started 
for  instance  in  the  skin,  would  under  normal  conditions  lead  to 
sensations  of  touch,  of  heat  or  cold,  or  of  pain,  fail  to  effect 
consciousness,  when  they  originate  on  the  affected  side;  the 
patient  cannot  on  that  side  feel  a  rough  surface,  or  a  hot  body, 
or  the  prick  of  a  pin.  For  the  sake  of  clearness  we  suppose  the 
loss  of  movement  and  sensation  to  be  complete,  but  it  might  of 
course  be  partial.  Such  a  case  shews  we  repeat  that  the  integrity 
of  the  cerebral  hemisphere,  and  of  the  connections  of  that  hemi- 
sphere, we  may  say  of  the  cortex  of  that  hemisphere,  with  the 
other  parts  of  the  nervous  system,  is  essential  to  the  development 
of  the  sensations  ;  but  it  does  not  prove  that  the  cortex  of  the 
hemisphere  is  the  "seat"  of  the  sensations,  it  does  not  prove  that 
the  afferent,  and  sensory  impulses  started  in  the  skin,  undergo  no 
material  change  until  they  reach  the  cortex  and  are  then  suddenly 
converted  into  sensations;  it  only  proves  that  in  the  complex 
chain  of  events  by  which  sensory  impulses  give  rise  to  full  con- 


1094  CUTANEOUS   SENSATIONS.  [BOOK  HI. 

scious  sensations,  the  events  in  the  cortex  furnish  an  indispensable 
link.  And  the  phenomena  of  the  dog  in  question  on  the  one  hand 
illustrate  how  complex  the  chain  is,  and  on  the  other  hand  suggest 
that  the  completeness  of  the  loss  of  sensation  in  the  hemiplegic 
man  is  not  a  pure  "deficiency"  phenomenon,  but  is  due  to  the 
lesion  affecting  the  chain  of  events  in  some  way  or  other  besides 
merely  removing  the  link  furnished  by  means  of  the  cortex.  For 
as  we  previously  urged,  the  dog  in  question,  however  curtailed  its 
psychical  life  may  have  been,  seemed  to  a  casual  observer  to  feel 
and  move  much  as  usual.  Neglecting  visual  and  auditory  sensa- 
tions with  which  we  are  not  now  dealing,  it  needed  careful 
observation  to  ascertain  that  some  of  the  animal's  movements  fell 
short,  the  failure  being  apparently  due  to  the  lack  of  adequately 
energetic  coordinating  sensory  impulses ;  a  stronger  stimulus  than 
usual  had  to  be  applied  to  the  skin  in  order  to  call  forth  the  usual 
movements  and  other  tokens  that  the  stimulus  was  "felt."  As 
we  have  before  urged,  it  is  impossible  to  suppose  that  the  mere 
stump  of  cerebrum  left  in  this  case  could  have  taken  on  all  the 
functions  of  the  lost  hemispheres ;  and  making  as  we  have  pre- 
viously done  full  allowance  for  the  differentiation  between  man 
and  dog,  we  must  conclude  that  in  the  more  general  sensations 
with  which  we  are  now  dealing,  as  with  the  more  special  visual 
sensations,  the  full  development  of  a  complete  sensation  is  a 
complex  act  of  more  stages  than  one  between  the  afferent  impulse 
along  the  afferent  nerve  and  the  affection  of  consciousness  which 
we  subjectively  recognise  as  'the  sensation;'  the  cortical  events 
are  only  some  among  several.  It  follows  that  any  analogy 
between  the  cortical  events  which  play  their  part  in  a  sensation 
and  the  cortical  events  which  immediately  precede  the  issue  of 
impulses  from  the  motor  region  along  the  fibres  of  the  pyramidal 
load  is  misleading;  the  highly  differentiated  motor  localisation 
does  not  justify  us  in  concluding  that  there  exists  a  similar 
topographical  distribution  of  sensations. 

§  680.  We  may  now  attack  the  problem  in  a  different  way, 
and  instead  of  beginning  with  the  cortex  begin  with  afferent 
impulses  started  along  afferent  nerves  from  their  peripheral 
endings,  and  attempt  to  trace  them  centralwards.  And  first  we 
may  call  to  mind  what  anatomical  guidance  we  possess.  (§  569.) 

We  have  seen  that  the  fibres  of  posterior  roots,  the  channels  of 
afferent  impulses,  end  in  the  spinal  cord  in  at  least  two  main  ways. 
One  set  are  continued  on,  not  broken  by  any  relays,  as  the  median 
posterior  tract,  and  by  this  tract  representatives  of  all  the  spinal 
nerves  are  connected  with  the  gracile  nucleus  in  which,  §  610,  the 
median  posterior  column  ends.  The  other  fibres  of  a  posterior 
root  appear  to  end  in  the  grey  matter  not  far  from  their  entrance ; 
but  from  the  grey  matter  there  starts  the  cerebellar  tract,  which 
though  not  conclusively  proved  to  be,  may  be  assumed  to  be  an 
afferent  tract.  WTe  may  therefore  probably  suppose  that  afferent 


CHAP,  ii.]  THE   BRAIN.  1095 

impulses  along  certain  of  the  fibres  of  the  posterior  root  make 
their  way  upwards  along  the  cerebellar  tract,  and  there  are  some 
reasons  for  regarding  the  vesicular  cylinder  and  the  cells  which 
represent  this  where  it  is  not  conspicuous  in  the  regions  of  the  cord, 
as  a  relay  between  the  two  systems  of  fibres.  There  are  also  the 
more  scattered  fibres  of  the  ascending  antero-lateral  tract  (§  567), 
which  probably  is  also  an  afferent  tract,  and  therefore  probably 
also  connected  with  the  posterior  roots ;  but  as  we  have  seen  our 
knowledge  of  this  tract  is  imperfect,  though,  if  as  some  urge  it 
ends  in  the  restiform  body,  we  may  perhaps  consider  it  as  similar 
at  least  to  the  cerebellar  tract,  and  treat  the  two  as  one. 

Thus  there  seem  to  be  at  least  two  main  recognised  paths,  in 
the  form  of  tracts  of  fibres,  for  afferent  impulses  along  the  cord ; 
one  along  the  median  posterior  column,  the  other  along  the  lateral 
column  in  the  cerebellar  tract.  The  latter  passes  straight  up  to 
the  cerebellum  by  the  restiform  body,  travelling  along  the  same 
side  of  the  cord ;  and  any  crossing  of  impulses  passing  along  this 
tract  must  take  place  before  they  enter  the  tract ;  we  have  how- 
ever no  anatomical  guidance  for  such  a  crossing.  The  other  path, 
along  the  median  posterior  tract,  comes  to  end  in  the  gracile 
nucleus;  it  has  indeed  been  urged  that  the  gracile  nucleus  is 
thus  connected  chiefly  with  the  lower  limbs  and  lower  part  of 
the  body,  and  that  the  analogous  posterior  root  fibres  from  the 
upper  limbs  and  neck  pass  similarly  into  the  cuneate  nucleus,  or 
at  least  into  the  median  division  of  that  nucleus,  but  this  cannot 
be  considered  as  proved.  Moreover  both  the  posterior  columns, 
median  and  external,  bring  to  these  nuclei  fibres  which  have 
started  from  some  relay  in  the  grey  matter  lower  down,  and 
which  are  not  fibres  coming  straight  without  any  relay  from 
the  posterior  roots;  these  however  we  cannot  distinguish  from 
each  other  in  their  course  beyond  the  nuclei.  From  the  gracile 
and  cuneate  nuclei  the  path  onward  is  a  double  one,  one  broad, 
one  narrow.  The  broad  path,  the  one  having  most  fibres  and 
presumably  carrying  most  impulses,  leads  to  the  cerebellum  by 
the  restiform  body;  and  here  the  path,  previously  continued 
exclusively  along  the  same  side  of  the  cord,  becomes  partly 
crossed  though  remaining  partly  uncrossed,  the  sensory  decussa- 
tion  in  the  bulb  being  the  crossed  and  the  other  fibres  passing 
from  the  nuclei  straight  to  the  restiform  body  being  the  uncrossed 
one  (§  612) ;  the  uncrossed  one  we  may  perhaps  look  upon  as 
really  an  upper  part  of  the  cerebellar  tract.  The  narrow  path  is 
the  fillet  (§  634),  by  which  some  of  the  fibres  from  the  nuclei  are 
continued  on  towards  the  cerebrum.  This  path  is  a  crossed  one, 
the  crossing  taking  place  in  the  sensory  decussation,  and  it  carries 
relatively  few  impulses,  the  chief  increase  in  the  size  of  the  fillet 
as  it  passes  onward  being  due  to  fibres  coming  from  structures 
other  than  the  gracile  and  cuneate  nuclei. 

Hence  of  the  sensory  impulses   travelling   along  continuous 


1096  CUTANEOUS   SENSATIONS.  [BOOK  in. 

tracts  in  the  spinal  cord,  these  tracts  apparently  keeping  always 
to  the  same  side,  the  great  majority  pass  to  the  cerebellum ;  and 
of  these  again  the  greater  number,  all  those  along  the  cerebellar 
tract,  and  some  of  those  passing  through  the  gracile  and  cuneate 
nuclei  remain  uncrossed  to  the  end.  The  only  path  by  which 
all  these  impulses  thus  passing  to  the  cerebellum  can  gain 
access  to  the  cortex  of  the  cerebrum,  is  by  some  or  other  of  the 
ties  between  the  cerebellum  and  the  cerebral  cortex.  The  rela- 
tively few  impulses  which  pass  along  the  fillet  are  for  the  most 
part  landed  in  the  middle  parts  of  the  brain,  for  only  a  small 
portion  of  the  fillet  passes  to  the  cortex  (§  634),  and  it  is  not 
clear  that  this  part  of  the  fillet  comes  from  the  gracile  and 
cuneate  nuclei,  so  that  most  of  these  impulses  can  gain  access  to 
the  cortex  only  by  the  relays  of  these  middle  parts  of  the  brain. 

Very  striking  indeed  are  these  constant  relays  along  the  path 
of  sensory  impulses ;  in  this  respect  the  sensory  impulses  offer  a 
strong  contrast  to  the  motor  impulses.  But  a  still  more  complex 
system  of  relays  has  to  be  mentioned ;  for  yet  a  third  path  is  open 
for  sensory,  afferent  impulses  along  the  cord.  We  must  admit  the 
possibility  of  afferent  impulses  travelling  along  the  network  of  the 
grey  matter,  their  path  being  either  absolutely  confined  to  the 
grey  matter,  or  leaving  the  grey  matter  at  intervals,  and  joining 
it  again  by  means  of  those,  longer  or  shorter,  commissural  or 
internuncial  fibres  which  unite  the  longitudinal  segments  of  grey 
matter,  arid  form  no  inconsiderable  portion  of  the  whole  white 
matter  of  the  cord.  We  have  seen  (§  586)  that  under  abnormal 
circumstances,  impulses  pass  freely  in  all  directions  along  the  grey 
matter,  and  we  may  conclude  that  under  normal  circumstances 
they  can  pass  along  it,  under  restrictions  and  along  lines  deter- 
mined by  physiological  conditions.  The  fibres  in  the  white  matter 
which  do  not  shew  either  descending  or  ascending  degeneration 
are,  probably,  as  we  have  said  (§  581),  internuncial  fibres,  con- 
necting segments  of  grey  matter  in  a  longitudinal  direction ;  and, 
though  we  have  no  exact  knowledge  touching  this  matter,  we  may 
suppose  that  some  of  these  convey  impulses  upwards,  and  others 
downwards. 

If,  as  some  maintain,  the  fibres  of  the  ascending  antero-lateral 
tract  end  not  in  the  cerebellum,  but  in  the  grey  matter  of  the 
bulb,  or  higher  up,  we  have  a  fourth  path  for  sensory  impulses, 
which  after  the  primary  relay  in  the  segmental  grey  matter  pass 
straight  up  to  the  bulb. 

§  681.  How  do  experimental  results  and  clinical  histories 
accord  with  such  an  anatomical  programme  ? 

We  may  first  call  attention  to  an  experiment,  which  though 
somewhat  old,  carried  out  on  rabbits,  and  confined  to  one  region 
only  of  the  cord,  the  lower  thoracic,  has  nevertheless  a  certain  value 
on  account  of  its  affording  more  or  less  distinctly  quantitative  and 
measurable  results.  We  have  seen,  §  175,  that  afferent  impulses 


CHAP,  ii.]  THE   BRAIN.  1097 

started  in  afferent  fibres,  in  those  for  instance  of  the  sciatic  nerve, 
so  affect  the  vaso-motor  centre  in  the  bulb  as  to  cause  a  rise  of 
blood-pressure,  at  least  in  an  animal  under  urari.  Those  afferent 
impulses  must  pass  by  some  path  or  other  from  the  roots  which 
supply  the  sciatic  nerves  with  afferent  fibres  along  the  thoracic 
and  cervical  cord  to  the  bulb.  If  the  path  be  blocked,  the 
stimulation  of  the  sciatic  nerve  will  fail  to  produce  the  usual  rise 
of  blood-pressure.  Now  in  a  rabbit,  the  amount  of  rise  of  blood- 
pressure  following  upon  the  stimulation  of  one  sciatic  nerve  with 
a  certain  strength  of  current  having  been  ascertained,  it  is  found 
that  a  much  less  rise  of  blood-pressure  or  none  at  all  follows  the 
same  stimulation  after  division  of  certain  parts  of  the  cord  in  the 
mid  or  upper  thoracic  region ;  that  is  to  say,  the  section  of  the 
cord  has  partially  or  completely  blocked  the  path  of  the  afferent 
impulses.  Further,  the  block  is  conspicuous  when  the  lateral 
column  is  divided,  and  is  not  increased  by  other  parts  of  the  cord 
being  divided  at  the  same  time ;  when  both  lateral  columns  are 
divided  the  block  is  almost  complete.  And  further,  supposing 
one  sciatic,  say  the  right,  is  the  one  which  is  stimulated,  a  block 
occurs  both  when  the  lateral  column  of  the  same,  right,  side  and 
when  that  of  the  crossed,  left,  side  is  divided,  but  is  greater  when 
the  division  is  on  the  crossed  than  when  it  is  on  the  same  side. 
We  may  infer  that  the  impulses,  which  reach  the  lumbar  cord  by 
the  roots  of  the  sciatic  nerve,  travel  up  the  cord,  or  give  rise 
within  the  lumbar  cord  to  events  which  we  may  compare  to 
nervous  impulses,  and  which  travel  up  the  cord  in  such  a  manner 
that  in  the  lower  thoracic  region  they  pass  almost  exclusively 
along  the  fibres  of  the  lateral  column,  some  having  kept  to  the 
same  side  of  the  cord,  but  more  having  crossed  over  to  the 
opposite  side,  before  reaching  the  thoracic  region. 

This  result  was  obtained  in  rabbits,  and  the  experiment  was 
carried  out  in  the  lower  thoracic  region  only ;  the  conclusions  to 
be  drawn  from  it  hold  good  for  that  animal  only,  and  for  that  part 
only  of  its  cord.  Moreover,  the  experiment  only  tests  the  path  of 
such  impulses  as  reach  and  affect  the  vaso-motor  centre  in  the 
bulb.  It  is  however  exceedingly  probable  that  the  impulses  which, 
generated  in  sensory  nerves,  affect  the  vaso-motor  centre  are 
impulses  which,  in  the  conscious  animal,  give  rise  to  sensations 
of  pain ;  in  an  intact  animal  changes  in  the  vaso-motor  centre 
occasioned  by  the  stimulation  of  sensory  nerves  are  accompanied 
by  signs  of  more  or  less  pain.  And  indeed  this  is  confirmed  by 
the  fact  that  similar  results  were  obtained  when,  the  experiment 
being  conducted  in  a  similar  way,  signs  of  pain  instead  of 
variations  in  blood-pressure  were  taken  as  the  tokens  of  the 
blocking  of  impulses.  Hence,  assuming  this,  we  may  regard  the 
experiment  as  indicating  that  the  impulses  which  form  the  basis 
of  painful  sensations  pass  by  the  lateral  columns  in  the  lower 
thoracic  region  of  the  cord  of  the  rabbit,  and  therefore,  though 


1098  CUTANEOUS   SENSATIONS.  [BOOK  HI. 

this  is  a  further  assumption,  by  the  same  columns  along  the 
whole  length  of  the  cord.  We  further  may  infer  that  while  some 
of  the  impulses  keep  to  the  same  side  of  the  cord,  others,  and 
indeed  the  greater  number,  cross  to  the  opposite  side. 

These  conclusions  entail  assumptions,  but  the  main  interpre- 
tation of  the  whole  experiment  entails  a  still  greater  assumption. 
The  testing  of  the  influence  of  the  sciatic  stimulation  was  carried 
out  soon  after  the  section  of  the  cord,  and  yet  we  have  assumed 
that  the  block  of  the  impulses  was  due  to  a  pure  deficiency 
phenomenon,  the  absence  of  a  usual  path.  But  we  have  no 
right  to  do  this.  It  is  possible  that  the  section  produced,  in 
some  way  or  other,  a  depressing  or  inhibitory  effect  lower  down 
in  the  cord,  affecting  structures  other  than  the  lateral  columns; 
all  our  experience  indeed  of  the  effects  of  operations  on  the  cord 
would  lead  us  to  expect  this.  It  is  further  possible  that  a  section 
of  the  lateral  column  might  produce  this  depressing  effect,  while 
sections  of  other  parts  did  not,  or  might  produce  more  effect  than 
they  could.  It  is  possible  for  instance  that  the  section  of  the 
thoracic  lateral  column  inhibited,  for  the  period  during  which  the 
experiment  was  carried  out,  the  grey  matter  of  the  lumbar  cord 
and  that  the  block  really  took  place  in  this  grey  matter.  Until 
the  uncertainties  thus  attending  the  interpretation  are  removed 
the  experiment  is  not  valid  as  a  proof  that  the  lateral  columns 
are  the  paths  of  afferent  impulses ;  it  would,  however,  still 
serve  to  indicate  that  the  afferent  impulses  reaching  the  cord 
along  the  sciatic  nerve  crossed  over  to  a  large  extent  before  they 
came  under  the  influence  of  the  inhibition,  since  we  have  no 
evidence  to  shew  that  such  an  inhibitory  action  of  the  section 
would  be  exerted  chiefly  on  the  crossed  side. 

Again,  we  have  seen  that  the  afferent  impulses  affecting  the 
vaso-motor  centre  gain  access  to  that  centre  without  the  help  of 
the  parts  of  the  brain  above  the  bulb ;  the  existence  of  the  vaso- 
motor  centre  was  made  out,  §  176,  by  combining  stimulation  of  a 
sciatic  nerve  with  a  series  of  operations  consisting  in  making 
successive  transverse  sections  of  the  bulb  from  above  downwards ; 
and  it  was  not  until  the  sections  reached  the  vaso-motor  centre 
that  the  blood-pressure  effects  of  the  sciatic  stimulation  were 
modified.  Hence  if  the  experiment  be  taken  as  shewing  that  not 
only  afferent  impulses  affecting  the  vaso-motor  centre,  but  other 
afferent  impulses  also  travel  by  the  lateral  columns,  it  would  also 
seem  to  shew  that  these  other  impulses  pass  in  like  manner  to  the 
bulb,  and  gain  access  to  the  cortex  through  the  bulb.  This 
increases  a  difficulty  which  presents  itself  even  when  the  afferent 
impulses  affecting  the  vaso-motor  centre  are  alone  considered. 
If  the  experiment  means  anything,  it  means  that  the  impulses 
having  in  some  way  or  other  reached  the  lateral  column,  travel  up 
that  column  by  some  continuous  path,  and  indeed  is  generally 
taken  as  having  that  meaning.  But  if  we  put  aside  the  very 


CHAP,  ii.]  THE   BRAIN.  1099 

doubtful  view  that  the  ascending  antero-lateral  tract  ends  in  the 
bulb,  there  is  no  continuous  afferent  tract  in  the  lateral  column 
ending  in  the  bulb ;  the  only  definite  continuous  afferent  tract  in 
the  lateral  column  of  which  we  have  any  clear  knowledge,  namely 
the  cerebellar  tract,  ends  not  in  the  bulb  but  in  the  cerebellum. 
And  if  we  attempt  to  get  out  of  the  difficulty  by  supposing  that 
those  impulses  at  least  which  affect  the  vaso-motor  centre,  after 
travelling  for  some  distance  in  the  cerebellar  tract,  leave  that 
tract  for  some  path  leading  to  the  bulb  (and  the  cerebellar  tract 
does  probably  give  off  as  well  as  receive  fibres  along  its  course), 
we  practically  admit  that  the  experiment  does  not  prove  the 
existence  of  a  continuous  path. 

A  further  difficulty  is  raised  by  the  fact  that,  according  to  the 
interpretation  which  we  are  discussing,  the  section  of  the  lateral 
column  breaks  the  paths  of  what  we  may  consider  two  kinds  of 
impulses ;  those,  the  larger  number,  which  have  already  crossed 
from  one  side  of  the  cord  to  the  other,  and  those  which  have 
remained  on  the  same  side.  For,  as  we  have  already  said,  we 
have  evidence,  in  man  at  least  and  some  other  animals,  that 
afferent  impulses  cross  completely  over  somewhere  or  other  on 
their  path  before  they  are  developed  into  full  sensations ;  and  we 
have  also  evidence,  though  less  strong,  that  they  cross  not  long 
after  their  entrance  into  the  cord.  But,  if  we  suppose  this  to  be 
the  case  in  the  rabbit  also,  it  follows  that  in  the  experiment  in 
question  the  impulses  which  were  blocked  on  their  passage  along 
the  lateral  column  of  the  same  side,  whatever  the  way  by  which 
they  reached  that  lateral  column,  were  pursuing  a  path  which 
would  eventually  have  led  them  to  the  other  side  of  the  cord. 
Hence  the  section  of  the  lateral  column,  in  breaking  their  path, 
broke  not  a  continuous  path  keeping  to  the  lateral  column  up  the 
length  of  the  cord,  but  a  path  which  soon  left  the  lateral  column 
to  pass  elsewhere.  The  experiment  therefore,  as  far  as  the 
impulses  passing  up  the  same  side  are  concerned,  does  not  prove 
that  they  pursue  a  continuous  path  along  the  lateral  column : 
and  if  so  what  becomes  of  the  validity  of  the  experiment  as  regards 
the  impulses  crossing  over  from  the  other  side,  for  the  experiment 
in  itself  makes  no  distinction  between  the  two  ? 

We  may  add  however  that  though  the  point  has  not  been 
specially  investigated,  it  is  possible  that  in  the  rabbit,  in  whose 
hind  limbs  bilateral  movements  are  so  predominant,  there  is 
associated  with  the  movements  a  bilateral  arrangement  for 
sensations,  and  that  those  impulses  which  remain  along  the 
same  side  of  the  cord  as  the  nerve  in  which  they  originate, 
are  carried  up  to  the  brain  without  any  crossing  at  all. 

§  682.  The  results  of  this  vaso-motor  experiment  then,  though 
they  are  frequently  quoted,  do  not  when  closely  considered  afford 
adequate  proof  that  afferent  impulses  pursue  a  continuous  path 
along  the  lateral  columns  of  the  cord,  and  moreover  the  facts 


1100  CUTANEOUS   SENSATIONS.  [BOOK  m. 

brought  to  light  by  the  experiment  shew  but  little  accord  with 
the  anatomical  programme.  We  have  dwelt  on  it  so  long  because 
it  is  more  or  less  illustrative  of  the  many  difficulties  attending 
the  interpretation  of  experiments  of  this  kind ;  and  it  is  in  this 
respect  all  the  more  valuable  because  the  actual  experimental 
results  are  sharp  and  clear.  We  may  pass  over  more  rapidly  the 
numerous  experiments  on  the  lower  mammals,  such  as  rabbits 
and  dogs,  in  which  other  indications  of  sensation  have  been  made 
use  of,  chiefly  those  which  are  the  signs  of  painful  sensations  ;  these 
have  been  carried  out  in  various  regions  of  the  cord,  but  chiefly 
in  the  thoracic  region,  and  in  them  a  like  uncertainty  of  inter- 
pretation is  farther  increased  by  the  want  of  exactness  and 
agreement  in  the  results. 

If  we  content  ourselves  with  making  no  distinction  between 
the  different  kinds  of  afferent  impulses,  and  in  the  case  of  these 
animals  it  would  hardly  be  profitable  to  attempt  to  make  a 
distinction,  we  may  say  that  the  several  experiments  so  far  agree 
that  they  point  to  the  lateral  columns  as  being  the  chief  paths  of 
afferent,  sensory,  impulses,  or  to  speak  more  exactly,  to  the 
passage  of  these  impulses  being  especially  blocked  by  section  of 
the  lateral  columns.  Some  observers  find  that  in  the  dog  and 
other  lower  mammals  a  section  of  the  lateral  column  on  one  side, 
or  at  least  a  hemisection  of  the  cord,  produces  ( loss  of  sensa- 
tion' on  the  opposite  side  greater  than  on  the  same  side,  or 
confined  to  the  opposite  side,  and  even  accompanied  by  an 
exaltation  of  sensation,  a  hyperesthesia,  on  the  same  side.  Other 
observers  again,  and  these  certainly  competent  observers,  find  that, 
in  the  dog,  section  of  one  side  affects  sensation  on  both  sides,  and 
indeed  chiefly  on  the  same  side.  We  may  perhaps  once  more 
repeat  the  warning  how  difficult  is  the  quantitative  and  qualitative 
determination  of  sensations  in  such  an  animal  as  the  dog ;  and 
may  remark  that  in  all  these  cases  of  unilateral  section  the 
increased  blood  supply  due  to  failure  of  the  normal  vaso-con- 
strictor  tone  must  influence  the  peripheral  development  of 
sensory  impulses. 

In  these  experiments,  as  in  those  on  voluntary  movements,  it 
is  most  important  to  distinguish  between  immediate  or  temporary 
and  more  lasting  effects ;  and  observers  have  found  that  the  loss 
of  sensation  following  a  hemisection  of  the  cord,  like  the  loss  of 
voluntary  movement,  is  temporary  only,  and  eventually  disappears, 
though  the  recovery  is  slower  and  less  complete  than  is  the  case 
with  movements.  As  with  voluntary  movement  (§  663)  so  with 
sensation,  recovery,  though  less  complete  than  that  of  movement, 
is  possible  when  a  hemisection  on  one  side  has  been  at  a  later 
date  followed  by  a  hemisection  on  the  other  side.  We  may 
therefore  repeat  in  reference  to  sensations  the  remarks  which 
we  then  made  in  reference  to  movement ;  there  is  however  an 
important  difference  between  the  two  cases ;  in  respect  to  move- 


CHAP,  ii.]  THE   BRAIN.  1101 

ment  we  have  evidence  that  under  normal  conditions  the  pyramidal 
tract  plays  an  important  part,  and  that  any  other  path  for  volitional 
impulses  is  more  or  less  an  alternative  one,  whereas  in  respect  to 
sensation  we  have  no  anatomical  or  other  distinct  proof  of  any 
such  normal  path. 

The  experiments  on  monkeys  are  in  like  manner  neither 
accordant  nor  decisive ;  and  even  in  these  animals  with  their 
more  varied  signs  of  sensations,  the  interpretation  of  these  signs 
is  beset  with  fallacies.  Some  observers  have  found  that  a  hemi- 
section  (in  the  thoracic  region)  produced  loss  of  sensation  on  the 
crossed  side,  accompanied  by  little  or  no  loss  on  the  same  side ; 
other  observers  again  have  failed  to  obtain  after  a  hemisection 
satisfactory  proof  of  any  such  marked  loss  on  the  crossed  side. 
Further,  large  portions  of  the  lateral  column,  the  more  internal 
parts  adjacent  to  the  grey  matter  being  left,  have  been  removed 
without  any  very  obvious  and  certainly  without  any  lasting 
defects  of  sensation  on  the  one  side  or  on  the  other. 

§  683.  The  clinical  histories  of  diseases  of  the  spinal  cord  in 
man  bring  to  light  in  a  fairly  clear  manner  a  fact  of  some  import- 
ance, namely,  that  the  several  impulses  which  form  the  bases  of 
the  several  kinds  of  sensations,  of  touch,  heat,  cold,  and  pain,  and 
of  the  muscular  sense,  are  transmitted  along  the  cord  in  different 
ways  and  presumably  by  different  structures.  For  disease  may 
impair  one  of  these  sensations  and  leave  the  others  intact.  Thus 
cases  of  spinal  disease  are  recorded,  in  which  on  one  side  of  the 
body  or  in  one  limb  ordinary  tactile  sensations  seemed  to  be  little 
impaired,  and  yet  sensations  of  pain  were  absent ;  when  a  needle 
was  thrust  into  the  skin  no  pain  was  felt,  though  the  patient  was 
aware  that  the  needle  has  been  pressed  upon  the  skin  at  a 
particular  spot;  and  conversely  in  other  cases  pain  has  been 
felt  upon  the  insertion  of  a  needle,  though  mere  contact  with 
or  pressure  on  the  skin  could  not  be  appreciated.  Again,  cases 
are  recorded  in  which  the  skin  was  sensitive  to  touch  or  pain, 
but  not  to  variations  of  temperature ;  it  is  farther  stated  that 
cases  have  been  met  with  in  which  cold  could  be  appreciated  but 
not  heat,  and  vice  versa ;  and  there  are  some  facts  which  point  to 
sensations  of  pain  being  more  closely  associated  with  those  of 
heat,  and  tactile  sensations  with  those  of  cold,  than  those  of  pain 
with  those  of  touch  or  those  of  heat  with  those  of  cold.  Cases  of 
spinal  disease  are  also  recorded  in  which  the  muscular  sense 
appeared  to  be  affected  apart  from  other  sensations.  We  shall 
return  to  these  matters  later  on  in  dealing  with  the  senses ;  we 
refer  to  them  now  simply  as  shewing  that  disease,  limited  as  far 
as  can  be  ascertained  to  the  spinal  cord,  may  affect  the  several 
sensations  separately,  and  therefore  as  suggesting  that  the  several 
kinds  of  impulses,  forming  the  bases  of  the  several  kinds  of 
sensation,  are  transmitted  in  different  ways  and  follow  different 
"paths"  along  the  spinal  cord. 


1102  CUTANEOUS   SENSATIONS.  [BOOK  m. 

Clinical  histories  moreover  agree,  at  least  to  large  extent,  in 
shewing  that  when  the  lesion  is  confined  to  one  half  of  the  cord, 
the  sensations  affected  in  the  parts  below  the  level  of  the  lesion  are 
chiefly  or  even  exclusively  those  of  the  crossed  side.  But  there  is 
not  entire  accordance,  especially  as  to  the  crossing  being  complete. 
And  with  regard  to  the  muscular  sense  there  is  a  distinct  conflict 
of  opinion ;  the  majority  of  cases  seem  to  shew  that  in  unilateral 
disease  or  injury  to  the  cord,  the  muscular  sense  in  company  with 
the  voluntary  movements,  fails  on  the  same  side ;  but  cases  have 
been  recorded  in  which  the  muscular  sense  in  company  with  other 
sensations,  seemed  to  be  affected  on  the  crossed  side ;  it  must  be 
remembered  however  that  it  is  very  difficult  to  appreciate  a 
deficiency  of  muscular  sense  mingled  with  deficiencies  in  other 
sensations,  and  we  should  a  priori  expect  the  muscular  sense  to 
run  parallel  with  motor  impulses. 

When  however  we  appeal  to  clinical  histories  or  indications  as 
to  the  several  paths  within  the  spinal  cord  taken  by  these  several 
impulses,  the  answer  is  a  most  uncertain  one,  as  indeed  might  be 
expected  from  the  too  often  diffuse  character  of  the  lesions  of 
disease ;  and  it  is  perhaps  not  too  much  to  say  that  no  satisfactory 
deductions  at  all  can  be  made. 

§  684.  Whether  then  we  turn  to  experiments  on  animals  or 
to  the  study  of  disease,  the  teachings  with  regard  to  sensation,  in 
contrast  to  those  with  regard  to  voluntary  movement,  are  in  the 
highest  degree  uncertain  and  obscure.  A  few  general  reflections 
will  perhaps  help  us  to  appreciate  the  value  of  such  facts  as  we 
possess. 

We  have  seen  reason  to  think  that  in  every  movement  whether 
voluntary  and  of  cortical  origin,  or  involuntary  and  started  either 
as  a  simple  spinal  reflex  or  through  the  working  of  some  part 
or  other  of  the  brain,  the  motor  impulses,  which  sweep  down  the 
motor  fibres  to  the  muscles,  issue  marshalled  and  coordinated  from 
the  grey  matter  of  the  cord  (for  the  sake  of  clearness  we  may  omit 
the  cranial  nerves),  from  what  we  have  called  the  motor  mechan- 
isms of  the  cord.  Analogy  would  lead  us  to  suppose  that  the 
afferent  impulses,  forming  the  bases  of  the  several  kinds  of  sensa- 
tions, similarly  left  the  afferent  fibres  to  join  the  grey  matter  of 
the  cord  in  what  we  may  call  the  sensory  mechanism.  And  such 
anatomical  leading  as  we  possess  seems  to  support  this  view  ;  with 
the  exception  of  the  median  posterior  tract,  to  which  we  will  return 
immediately,  all  the  fibres  of  a  posterior  root  seem  to  end  in  the 
grey  matter  not  very  far  from  the  entrance  of  the  root.  We  have 
seen  that  a  coordinate  reflex  movement  may  be  carried  out  by  at 
least  a  few  segments  of  the  cord ;  that  a  reflex  movement  may 
be  started  by  stimuli  of  various  kinds  and  therefore  presumably 
by  afferent  impulses  of  various  kinds ;  and  that  impulses  forming 
the  basis  of  the  muscular  sense  are  essential  to  the  coordination  of 
the  movement.  All  our  knowledge  goes  to  shew  that  in  a  reflex 


CHAP.  IL]  THE   BRAIN.  1103 

movement  carried  out  by  a  few  segments  of  the  cord,  the  whole 
chain  of  events  between  the  arrival  of  the  afferent  impulses  along 
the  posterior  root  and  the  issue  of  efferent  impulses  along  the 
anterior  root  may  be  carried  out  by  grey  matter,  and  grey  matter 
alone.  We  may  further  infer  that,  while  on  the  one  hand  the 
same  procedure  might  obtain  not  through  a  few  segments  only 
but  along  the  whole  length  of  the  cord,  there  would  be  an 
advantage,  especially  in  respect  to  the  rapidity  of  transmission, 
in  employing  internuncial  tracts  of  fibres  between  the  several 
segments,  the  advantage  being  greater  the  more  distant  the 
segments  which  have  to  work  together. 

We  might  further  suppose  that  it  would  be  of  advantage  to 
possess  some  direct  path  between  the  cerebral  cortex  and  the 
spinal  sensory  mechanism  immediately  connected  with  the  pos- 
terior root,  such  as  is  afforded  by  the  pyramidal  tract  between  the 
cortex  and  the  spinal  motor  mechanism  immediately  connected 
with  the  anterior  root.  But  no  anatomical  evidence  of  such  a  tract 
is  forthcoming;  and,  as  we  have  before  remarked,  along  all  the 
tracts  which  seem  to  be  sensory  in  nature,  in  contrast  to  what 
takes  place  in  the  motor  tracts,  relays  of  grey  matter  are  con- 
tinually being  interpolated. 

The  median  posterior  tract,  since  it  gathers  up  representatives 
of  successive  nerves,  presents  itself  as  the  nearest  approach  to 
such  a  sensory  homologue  of  the  pyramidal  tract,  though  it  ends 
in  the  bulb,  and  is  not  continued  on  directly  to  the  cortex.  And 
possibly  it  does  play  a  somewhat  analogous  part,  in  so  far  as  it 
serves  as  a  special  connection  between  the  brain  and  the  whole 
series  of  spinal  nerves.  But  we  are  wholly  ignorant  as  to  what  it 
really  does ;  and  whatever  be  the  exact  nature  of  the  part  which 
it  plays,  it  probably  has  relations  not  to  one  kind  of  sensation 
only,  but  to  all  the  different  kinds  of  sensation.  It  has  indeed 
been  supposed  by  some  to  be  especially  a  tract  for  the  impulses 
of  the  muscular  sense ;  but  neither  experiment  nor  clinical  study 
affords  adequate  proof  of  this  view.  The  condition  known  as 
locomotor  ataxy,  the  salient  feature  of  which  is  loss  or  impairment 
of  muscular  sense,  is  associated  with  disease  of  the  posterior  root 
and  of  its  entrance  into  the  cord,  not  with  disease  confined 
exclusively  to  the  median  posterior  column.  Moreover  the  tract 
cannot  carry  all  the  impulses  of  muscular  sense,  since  some  of 
them  must  pass  at  once  into  the  grey  matter,  to  take  part  in  the 
coordination  of  reflex  movements,  and  must  therefore  travel  by 
fibres  which  do  not  form  this  tract.  Similarly  is  there  no 
adequate  proof  of  the  tract  being  an  exclusive  channel  for 
tactile  or  for  painful  sensations. 

We  may  also  perhaps  urge  similar  considerations  with  regard 
to  the  cerebellar  tract,  which  though  starting  from  a  relay  of  grey 
matter  is  thence  onward  to  the  cerebellum  a  continuous  tract. 
This  tract  also  has  been  supposed  to  carry  impulses  of  a  par- 

F.  70 


1104  CUTANEOUS   SENSATIONS.  [BOOK  in. 

ticular  kind,  and  more  particularly  those  of  muscular  sense. 
There  is  less  a  priori  objection  to  this  view,  since  the  tract 
starts  from  the  grey  matter,  where  the  impulses  of  muscular  sense 
may  have  already  done  their,  so  to  speak,  local  work,  and  ends 
in  the  cerebellum,  which  as  we  have  seen  seems  especially 
connected  with  the  coordination  of  movements.  But  with  respect 
to  this  tract  also,  neither  experiment  nor  clinical  study  affords 
any  clear  and  decisive  proof  that  it  is  solely  or  even  especially 
concerned  with  the  muscular  sense. 

With  regard  to  the  antero-lateral  ascending  tract  our  know- 
ledge is  too  imperfect  to  justify  us  in  supposing  that  it  is  the 
special  or  exclusive  channel  for  any  one  kind  of  sensation,  or 
indeed  in  drawing  any  conclusions  at  all  concerning  it. 

But  when  we  subtract  from  the  white  matter  of  the  cord  these 
continuous  tracts  of  ascending  degeneration  of  presumably  sensory 
or  afferent  function,  and  the  continuous  tracts  of  descending 
degeneration,  which  we  may  confidently  speak  of  as  motor  or  at 
least  efferent,  there  are  left  only  the  fibres  which  we  have  (§  581), 
supposed  to  be  longitudinal  commissural  or  internuncial  fibres 
between  successive  segments.  We  are  thus  driven  back  to  our 
former  conclusion,  that  sensory  impulses  pass  either  by  the  grey 
matter  alone,  or  by  a  series  of  steps  as  it  were,  by  relays  of  grey 
matter  connected  by  internuncial  tracts  of  fibres,  whose  length  we 
cannot  ascertain,  but  which  may  be  short.  That  such  inter- 
nuncial tracts  intervene  is  rendered  probable,  on  the  one  hand 
by  the  fact  that  section  of  the  white  matter,  leaving  the  grey 
untouched,  does  affect  sensations,  and  on  the  other  hand  by  the 
fact  that  the  several  kinds  of  sensation  appear  to  travel  along  the 
cord  by  separate  paths,  or  at  least  may  be  separately  blocked. 
It  is  of  course,  as  we  have  already  urged,  possible  that  the  effect 
of  a  section  of  a  tract  of  fibres  may  be  not  the  mere  block  due  to 
loss  of  continuity,  but  some  action  on  the  grey  matter  with  which 
the  fibres  are  connected,  whereby  that  grey  matter  fails  of  its 
usual  functions  and  ceases  to  carry  onward  the  sensory  impulses 
reaching  it  from  below ;  it  is  also  possible  that  this  or  that  lesion 
of  disease  may,  directly  or  indirectly,  affect  particular  parts  of 
the  grey  matter  or  affect  the  grey  matter  in  a  particular  way,  so 
that  a  certain  kind  of  sensory  impulse,  and  none  other  is  blocked. 
On  the  other  hand  we  have  reason  to  think  that  the  rate  at  which 
impulses  travel  along  the  grey  matter  is  very  slow  compared  with 
that  along  nerve  fibres ;  and  in  the  struggle  for  life,  rapidity  of 
transmission  of  nervous  impulses  is  of  great  importance.  Hence 
the  view  that  internuncial  fibres  intervene  has  more  to  commend 
it ;  it  is  moreover  to  a  certain  extent  supported  by  clinical 
histories.  But,  if  we  accept  this  view,  we  must  at  the  same 
time  admit  that,  in  animals  at  least,  the  lines  provided  by  the 
internuncial  tracts  are  not  rigid,  that  within  limits  and  under 
circumstances  alternative  routes  are  possible. 


CHAP.  IL]  THE   BRAIN.  1105 

§  685.  We  may  here  perhaps  raise  once  more,  and  this  time 
more  pointedly  than  before,  the  doubt  whether  we  are  justified 
in  assuming,  as  we  generally  do  assume,  that  the  events  which 
take  place  in  the  fibres  connecting  relays  of  grey  matter  within 
the  central  nervous  system,  are  exactly  the  same  as  those  which 
take  place  in  the  fibres  of  nerves  outside  the  central  system, 
during  the  passage  of  what  we  call  a  nervous  impulse.  Most 
of  our  knowledge  of  a  nervous  impulse  has  been  gained  by  the 
study  of  the  motor  nerve  of  a  muscle-nerve  preparation.  Our 
knowledge  of  the  processes  in  afferent  nerves  is  much  more 
imperfect ;  but  there  are  many  facts  which  at  least  suggest  that 
the  molecular  events  constituting  an  afferent  impulse  along  an 
afferent  nerve  are  different  from,  and  probably  more  complicated 
than,  those  constituting  an  efferent  impulse  along  an  efferent  nerve. 
And,  with  regard  to  the  processes  taking  place  in  fibres  within 
the  central  nervous  system  we  have  hardly  any  exact  experimental 
knowledge  at  all.  It  has  been  maintained  by  many  observers  that 
not  only  the  grey  matter  but  also  the  tracts  of  white  matter  in 
the  spinal  cord,  while  they  are  capable  of  conveying  impulses  in 
one  direction  or  the  other,  are  incapable  of  being  so  excited  by 
artificial  stimuli  as  to  generate  new  impulses.  These  observers 
maintain  that,  when  movements  or  signs  of  sensation  follow  the 
direct  stimulation  of  various  parts  of  the  cord,  the  effects  are  due 
to  issuing  motor  fibres  or  entering  sensory  fibres  having  been 
stimulated,  and  not  to  a  stimulation  of  the  intrinsic  substance  of 
the  parts  themselves ;  they  propose  accordingly  to  call  these  parts 
"kinesodic"  and  "sesthesodic"  respectively,  that  is  to  say,  serving 
as  paths  for  motor  or  sensory  impulses  without  being  themselves 
either  motor  or  sensory.  The  evidence  on  the  whole  goes  to  shew 
that  this  view  is  a  mistaken  one,  that  the  various  tracts  of  the 
spinal  cord,  like  the  pyramidal  tract  and  indeed  other  parts  of  the 
brain,  are  excitable  towards  artificial  stimuli.  The  question 
cannot,  however,  be  considered  as  definitely  closed ;  and  the  very 
fact  that  it  has  been  raised  illustrates  the  point  on  which  we  are 
now  dwelling.  We  may  further  quote,  in  similar  illustration  of 
the  same  point,  the  following  remarkable  fact  which  was  observed 
in  the  series  of  experiments  referred  to  in  §  663  on  the  effects  of 
repeated  hemisection  of  the  spinal  cord  in  dogs.  The  animal  had 
partially  recovered  voluntary  movements  in  his  hind  limbs  after  a 
third  hemisection  of  the  thoracic  cord,  and  yet  when,  at  his  death, 
a  strong  tetanizing  current  was  directed  through  the  bulb  and 
cervical  cord,  no  movements  of  the  hind  limbs  followed:  the 
impulses  started  by  artificial  stimulation  could  not  pass  the  bridge 
which  sufficed  for  volitional  impulses  of  natural  origin.  It  is  not 
too  much  to  say  that  our  experimental  knowledge  as  to  the  events 
which  accompany  the  activity  of  the  structures  within  the  central 
nervous  system  is  almost  entirely  limited  to  the  recognition  of  the 
"currents  of  action"  referred  to  in  §  657.  We  are  already  going 

70—2 


1106  CUTANEOUS   SENSATIONS.  [BOOK  m. 

beyond  our  tether  when  we  assume  on  the  strength  of  this 
that  the  processes  started  in  the  fibres  of  the  pyramidal  tract  by 
artificial  stimulation  are  in  all  respects  identical  with  those  started 
in  the  fibres  of  a  motor  nerve.  We  are  going  still  more  beyond 
our  tether  when  we  assume  that  the  processes  started  in  the  same 
pyramidal  fibres  as  the  outcome  of  natural  events  in  the  motor 
cortex  are  of  the  same  kind.  But  these  assumptions  are  trifles 
compared  with  the  assumption  that  the  events  taking  place  in  the 
fibres  of  the  optic  radiation,  passing  from  the  pulvinar  to  the 
occipital  cortex  are  identical  with  the  events  taking  place  in  the 
fibres  of  the  optic  tract  on  the  way  to  the  pulvinar,  or  that  the 
events  travelling  along  the  spinal  cord  to  the  brain  as  the  result 
of  a  prick  of  the  little  finger  are  identical  with  those  which  the 
prick  has  started  in  the  fibres  of  the  ulnar  nerve.  Of  the  latter 
events  we  know  a  little ;  of  the  former  events  we  know  next  to 
nothing.  And  we  may  here  ask  the  question  what  is  the  meaning 
of  these  continual  relays  of  grey  matter  along  the  sensory  tract 
unless  it  be  that  at  each  relay,  some  transformation,  some  further 
elaboration  of  the  impulses  takes  place,  until  what  were  the 
relatively,  but  only  relatively,  simple  impulses  along  the  fibres  of 
the  peripheral  nerve  are  by  successive  steps  changed  in  the 
complex  events  which  we  call  a  conscious  sensation  ?  This  is 
what  we  had  in  mind,  when  we  gave  (§652)  a  note  of  warning 
concerning  the  danger  of  considering  all  the  events  in  the  central 
nervous  system  as  either  motor  or  sensory  in  nature.  It  is 
perhaps  not  an  exaggeration  to  represent  the  views  of  some 
observers  as  if  they  supposed  that  afferent  impulses,  say  tactile 
impulses,  that  is  impulses  eventually  giving  rise  to  tactile  sen- 
sations, travelled  unchanged  from  the  skin  to  the  cortex  and  there 
suddenly  blossomed  into  sensations.  If  such  a  view  were  true, 
undoubtedly  the  chief  task  of  physiology,  almost  the  only  one, 
would  be  to  ascertain  the  tract  along  which  these  impulses 
passed.  But  if  on  the  other  hand  the  views  just  now  urged 
have  any  real  foundation,  the  question  of  tracts  or  paths  sinks 
into  insignificance  compared  with  the  almost  untouched  problems 
as  to  what  are  the  several  changes  by  which  simple  impulses  are 
developed  into  full  sensations,  and  when  and  how  the  changes  are 
effected. 

§  686.  Seeing  how  unsatisfactory  is  our  present  knowledge 
with  regard  to  the  tracts  or  paths  of  sensations  in  the  relatively 
simple  spinal  cord,  it  would  be  useless  to  attempt  any  discussion 
as  to  their  paths  in  the  much  more  complex  brain.  If  it  be 
probable  that  the  passage  is  effected  by  relays  of  grey  matter 
in  the  former,  the  same  method  is  much  more  probable  in  the 
latter ;  and  if  neither  experiment  nor  clinical  study  throws  much 
light  on  the  path  up  to  the  bulb,  these  cannot  be  expected  to  give 
much  help  in  the  maze  of  grey  matter  and  fibres  by  which  the  bulb 
is  joined  to  the  cortex.  The  several  defined  areas  or  collections 


CHAP.  IL]  THE   BRAIN.  1107 

of  grey  matter,  and  the  several  strands  and  tracts  of  fibres  which 
we  briefly  described  in  a  previous  section,  must  have  of  course  a 
meaning ;  but  it  may  be  doubted  whether  we  have  even  so  much 
as  a  correct  glimpse  of  that  meaning  in  any  one  case,  if  we  except 
those  which  are  in  immediate  connection  with  the  cranial  nerves 
and  their  nuclei.  Seeing  that  the  thalamus  appears  on  the  one 
hand  to  be  connected  with  all  or  nearly  all  parts  of  the  cortex, 
and  on  the  other  hand  to  serve  as  the  front  of  the  tegmental 
system,  it  is  tempting  to  suppose  that  it  plays  an  important  part 
in  sensations  pertaining  to  the  body  generally,  as  part  of  it,  the 
pulvinar,  certainly  does  with  reference  to  the  special  sense  of 
sight;  but  we  have  no  decisive  indications  as  to  what  part  it  plays. 
And  the  part  which  it  plays,  whatever  that  may  be,  is  not  an 
exclusively  sensory  one,  since  both  experimental  and  morbid  lesions 
of  the  thalamus  are  apt  to  produce  disorders  of  movement  as  well 
as  other  efferent  effects.  We  ought  perhaps  to  say  the  parts 
which  it  plays;  for  it  is  a  complex  body,  having  many  ties  and 
probably  performing  many  duties. 

The  conspicuous  fillet  again,  seeming  as  it  does  to  be  a  special 
internuncial  tract  connecting  what  appear  to  be  more  particularly 
afferent  or  sensory  parts  of  the  bulb,  such  as  the  gracile  and 
cuneate  nuclei,  with  various  parts  of  the  middle  brain  and  pro- 
bably with  the  cortex,  presents  itself  as  a  probable  path  of 
sensations  of  one  kind  or  another  from  the  body  at  large,  the 
"narrow  path"  of  the  anatomical  programme  (§  680);  but  in 
reference  to  this  too,  beyond  its  probable  connection  with  the 
auditory  sensations  (§  677),  we  lack  evidence. 

A  conspicuous  part  of  the  brain,  namely  the  cerebellum, 
naturally  arrests  our  attention  on  account  of  its  large  connections 
with  what  appear  to  be  afferent  structures;  in  the  anatomical 
programme,  we  called  it  "the  broad  path."  By  the  cerebellar 
tract  it  has  an  uncrossed  grip  upon  what  is  practically  the 
whole  length  of  the  spinal  cord;  by  the  other  constituents  of 
the  inferior  peduncle  it  has  a  like  uncrossed  grip  upon  what 
appear  to  be  afferent  structures  in  the  bulb,  the  gracile  and 
cuneate  nuclei,  as  well  as  on  the  eighth  (vestibular)  nerve  and 
probably  representatives  of  other  afferent  cranial  nerves ;  it  has 
further  a  crossed  grip  through  the  gracile  and  cuneate  nuclei  on 
the  afferent  posterior  columns  of  the  whole  cord.  It  is  of  course 
possible  that  the  cerebellar  tract,  though  in  itself  uncrossed,  lays 
its  hand,  by  means  of  the  vesicular  cylinder  for  instance,  on 
impulses  which  have  already  crossed  from  the  posterior  roots 
of  the  other  side ;  for  as  we  have  seen  the  evidence  as  a  whole 
shews  that  sensory  impulses  do  cross  over;  but  neither  has  the 
crossing  of  the  impulses  been  definitely  proved,  nor  has  the  path 
of  the  crossing  been  clearly  demonstrated ;  while,  on  the  contrary, 
the  fibres  of  the  auditory  nerve  which  pass  to  the  cerebellum,  and 
which  as  we  have  suggested  (§  618),  may  be  compared  to  an 


1108  CUTANEOUS   SENSATIONS.  [BOOK  HI. 

outlying  part  of  the  cerebellar  tract,  certainly  continue  uncrossed 
into  the  peduncle  of  the  same  side.  We  may  conclude,  therefore, 
that  the  ties  of  the  cerebellum  with  the  posterior  roots  are  both 
crossed  and  uncrossed.  And  we  may  regard  this  double  grip  of  the 
cerebellum  on  the  cord,  this  grip  on  both  sides  of  it,  as  an  addi- 
tional evidence  that  the  ties  of  the  cerebellum  with  the  spinal  cord 
are  not  merely  for  the  purpose  of  serving  as  the  channel  for  the 
impulses  of  muscular  sense,  but  are  the  means  by  which  the  cere- 
bellum transforms  or  elaborates  sensory  impulses,  not  of  muscular 
sense  alone  or  chiefly,  but  probably  of  all  kinds,  in  order  that 
they  may  take  part  in  cerebral  operations,  of  which  the  coor- 
dination of  bodily  movements  may  be  one,  but  probably  is  only 
one  of  several  or  even  of  many. 


SEC.  10.     SOME  OTHER  ASPECTS  OF  THE  FUNCTIONS 
OF  THE  BRAIN. 


§  687.  It  is  difficult  to  say  anything  definite  concerning  the 
transmission  of  sensory  impulses  and  the  development  of  sensa- 
tions ;  it  is  still  more  difficult  to  say  anything  definite,  beyond  what 
has  been  already  incidentally  said,  concerning  the  parts  played 
in  the  work  of  the  brain  by  the  various  aggregations  of  grey 
matter  and  tracts  of  fibres  forming  the  middle  part  of  the  brain. 
Neither  experiment  nor  clinical  study  has  as  yet  afforded  any 
clear  or  sure  leading. 

To  what  has  already  been  said  about  the  cerebellum,  we  may 
add  the  following. 

Electrical  stimulation  of  the  surface  of  the  cerebellum,  in  the 
monkey  and  in  other  animals,  has  led  to  movements  of  the  eyes, 
and  of  other  parts  of  the  head :  but  we  cannot  from  such  results 
draw  any  satisfactory  inferences. 

The  removal  of  various  parts  of  the  cerebellum,  especially 
of  the  median  parts,  has  led  to  a  want  of  coordination  in  bodily 
movements ;  and  an  unsteady  gait,  due  to  a  like  want  of  ade- 
quate coordination,  is  a  frequent  symptom  of  cerebellar  disease. 
But  the  incoordination  which  makes  its  appearance  immediately 
after  removal  of,  or  injury  to,  the  cerebellum  may  eventually 
disappear,  even  though  large  portions  have  been  removed;  and 
many  cases  of  extensive  cerebellar  disease  have  been  recorded  in 
which  the  most  perfect  coordination  of  movements  was  retained. 
Hence  the  results  of  experimental  and  clinical  study,  while  on  the 
whole  supporting  the  conclusion  that  the  cerebellum  has  in  some 
way  to  do  with  coordination,  throw  little  or  no  light  on  the  exact 
nature  of  the  part  which  the  organ  plays  in  the  complex  process, 
but  perhaps  rather  shew  that  we  are  at  present  wholly  ignorant 
of  how  coordination  is  brought  about. 

Many  hypotheses  have  been  put  forward  as  to  the  work 
carried  out  by  the  cerebellum,  but  none  of  these  can  be  said  to 
have  an  adequate  basis.  And,  indeed,  if  there  be  any  value  in  the 
reflections  we  have  repeatedly  made  in  previous  pages,  the 
physiologist  ought  not  to  use  the  words  "functions  of  the 
cerebellum."  From  a  physiological  point  of  view  it  is,  so  to  speak, 


1110  THE   CEREBELLUM.  [BOOK  HI. 

a  matter  of  accident,  that  various  structures,  the  seats  of  various 
physiological  processes,  have,  from  morphological  causes,  been 
gathered  together  into  the  body  which  anatomists  call  the  cere- 
bellum. The  task  of  the  physiologist  is  to  unravel  the  ties 
binding  these  various  cerebellar  structures  with  other  parts  of 
the  central  nervous  system,  and  so  with  various  parts  of  the  body 
at  large. 

We  must  content  ourselves  here  with  calling  attention  to  two 
or  three  broad  and  suggestive  facts  concerning  its  structure  and 
connections. 

In  the  first  place,  one  striking  fact  about  the  cerebellum  is  the 
very  large  development  of  commissural  fibres  connecting  together 
the  superficial  grey  matter  of  the  two  hemispheres  for  the  greater 
part  of  their  extent,  and  passing,  not  only  through  the  pons  (§  635) 
as  part  of  the  middle  peduncle,  but  also  through  the  median 
vermis.  This  great  commissure  is  second  only  to  the  great 
callosal  commissure  of  the  cerebrum ;  and  from  the  fact  that 
median  lesions  of  the  cerebellum,  those  which  do  most  damage  to 
this  commissure,  are  the  most  effective  in  causing  incoordination 
and  forced  movements,  we  may  infer  that  it  in  some  way  plays 
an  important  part  in  coordination. 

A  second  striking  fact  is  one  on  which  we  have  already  just 
dwelt,  the  connection,  chiefly  an  uncrossed  one,  through  the 
inferior  peduncle,  with  the  afferent  structures  of  the  bulb  and 
spinal  cord.  We  may  now  add,  that  the  fibres  of  this  peduncle 
passing  into  the  centre  of  the  white  matter  of  the  cerebellar 
hemisphere  of  the  same  side  enclose  the  grey  matter  of  the 
nucleus  dentatus  and  appear  largely  to  end  in  that  body,  though 
some  pass  on  to  the  vermis. 

A  third  striking  fact  is  the  connection,  this  being,  as  far  as  we 
know,  wholly  a  crossed  one,  through  the  pons  and  pes,  with  the  cere- 
bral cortex,  both  of  the  extreme  frontal  region,  and  of  the  temporo- 
occipital  region,  and  possibly  or  even  probably  with  more  scattered 
cortical  elements  of  the  parietal  (motor)  region.  This  connection 
is  one  between  cortex  and  cortex,  or  at  least  between  cerebral 
cortex  and  cerebellar  superficial  grey  matter,  for  the  fibres  of  the 
middle  peduncle  passing  from  the  grey  matter  of  the  pons  which 
serves  as  a  relay  end  in  the  surface  of  the  lateral  hemisphere  of 
the  cerebellum.  The  frontal  cortical  fibres  passing  to  the  pes 
have  a  descending  degeneration,  that  is  from  the  cortex  to  the 
pons,  and  we  may  probably  assume  that  the  similar  temporo- 
occipital  fibres  similarly  degenerate  downwards  to  the  pons 
(§  632).  From  this  it  has  been  inferred  that  this  cerebro-cere- 
bellar  connection  carries  impulses  from  the  cerebral  cortex  to  the 
cerebellum ;  and  it  has  been  further  inferred  that  these  impulses 
are  of  the  nature  of  motor  impulses.  As  we  have  more  than  once 
urged,  the  character  of  degeneration,  that  is  whether  "ascending" 
or  "  descending"  is  not  a  satisfactory  proof  of  the  direction  taken 


CHAP.  IL]  THE   BRAIN.  1111 

by  impulses ;  but  it  is  perhaps  of  more  importance  to  remember 
that,  as  we  have  also  urged,  we  have  no  right  to  assume  that  the 
impulses  passing  along  such  a  tract  as  the  one  in  question  must 
be  either  sensory  or  motor,  or  indeed  that  such  a  tract  serves  as 
an  instrument  for  producing  effects  in  one  direction  only. 

That  during  life  the  fibres  of  which  we  are  speaking  serve  as 
an  important  chain  by  which  cerebral  cortex  and  cerebellum  affect 
the  one  the  other,  there  can  be  but  little  doubt;  but  we  are 
wholly  in  the  dark  as  to  what  really  takes  place  along  the  fibres. 
We  have  seen  (§  593)  reason  to  think  that  the  brain  may  and 
does  exert  an  inhibitory  influence  over  the  spinal  cord;  and  the 
mechanical  certainty  with  which  an  animal  deprived  of  its  cerebral 
hemispheres  responds  to  stimuli,  in  contrast  to  the  uncertainty 
attending  the  result  of  stimuli  applied  to  an  intact  animal,  as  well 
as  all  the  experience  of  our  own  daily  life  shews  that  the  cerebral 
cortex  can  work  in  an  inhibitory  manner  on  other  parts  of  the 
brain  ;  the  remarkable  "  forced  movements  "  on  which  we  dwelt  in 
a  previous  section  seem  in  some  instances  to  be  the  result  of  the 
abrupt  snap  of  some  inhibitory  bond.  Conversely  all  the  experi- 
ence of  our  daily  life,  many  of  the  phenomena  of  the  condition 
known  as  hypnotism  and  of  allied  conditions,  as  well  as  various 
experimental  results  such  as  that  quoted  in  §  661,  where  a 
sensory  impulse  seems  to  inhibit  the  activity  of  a  motor  area, 
shew  that  the  cortex  may  itself  in  turn  be  inhibited  by  other 
parts  of  the  central  nervous  system.  But  we  have  at  present  no 
satisfactory  indications  as  to  the  paths  of  inhibitory  impulses  or 
as  to  how  inhibition  is  brought  about ;  nor  have  we  any  proof  that 
the  cerebro-cerebellar  tract  is  an  inhibitory  one  in  either  direction. 

We  may  add  that  some  of  the  fibres  of  the  middle  peduncle 
appear  to  be  neither  commissural  nor  connected  with  the  cortical 
fibres  in  the  pes,  but  to  end  in  other  ways ;  and  tracts  have  been 
described  as  continuing  onwards  some  of  the  cerebellar  fibres  of 
the  middle  peduncle  on  the  one  hand  upwards  toward  the 
cerebrum,  and  on  the  other  hand  downwards  toward  the  spinal 
cord.  It  has  been  further  urged  that  these  tracts  are  efferent 
in  function. 

Lastly,  we  may  call  attention  to  the  superior  peduncles.  These, 
which  as  we  have  seen  appear  to  come  largely  from  the  grey 
matter  of  the  nucleus  dentatus  and  to  end  in  the  tegmentutn, 
largely  in  the  red  nucleus,  may  be  regarded  as  constituting 
through  the  relay  of  the  front  part  of  the  tegmentum  another 
tie,  presumably  of  a  different  nature  from  the  foregoing,  between 
the  cerebellum  and  the  cortex ;  indeed  it  used  to  be  called  the 
processus  a  cerebello  ad  cerebrum.  It  is  an  obviously  crossed 
tract  (Fig.  113,  $P);  it  connects  one  nucleus  dentatus,  and  so 
presumably  by  that  relay  the  fibres  of  the  inferior  peduncle 
ending  in  that  body,  and  perhaps  other  fibres  proceeding  from 
the  superficial  grey  matter  of  one  side  of  the  cerebellum,  with 


1112  THE   CORPORA   QUADRIGEMINA.        [BOOK  in. 

the  red  nucleus  and  other  parts  of  the  tegmentum  of  the  crossed 
side,  and  thus  with  the  cortex  of  the  crossed  side.  It  has  been 
supposed  that  the  direction  of  impulses  passing  along  it  is  from 
the  cerebrum  to  the  cerebellum,  but  we  have  no  clear  proof  of 
this ;  indeed  as  to  what  it  does,  we  have  no  satisfactory  evidence 
either  experimental  or  clinical. 

We  may  here  incidentally  remark  that,  in  consequence  of 
afferent  tracts  being  traced  to  or  towards  the  tegmentum  and 
of  the  sharp  contrast  presented  between  the  tegmentum  and 
the  conspicuously  motor  pyramidal  tract  in  the  pes,  the  view 
has  gained  ground  that  the  tegmentum  is  essentially  a  sensory 
structure.  But  there  does  not  appear  to  be  adequate  evidence 
either  clinical  or  experimental  for  such  a  conclusion.  The 
thalamus,  which  we  have  regarded  as  the  front  so  to  speak  of  the 
tegmentum,  cannot,  as  we  have  already  urged  (§  686),  be  con- 
sidered exclusively  or  especially  sensory.  And  many  of  the  ties 
of  the  tegmentum,  such  as  the  fibres  from  the  corpora  striata 
ending  in  the  substantia  nigra,  for  this  may  be  considered  as 
properly  belonging  to  the  tegmentum,  are  of  the  kind  which  we 
may  suppose  to  be  efferent  or  motor.  Indeed  we  may  probably 
regard  the  whole  tegmentum  as  being  broadly  the  analogue  in 
the  forward  segments  of  the  cerebro-spinal  axis  of  both  the 
anterior  and  posterior  grey  matter  of  the  spinal  segments  behind. 

Though  we  are  thus  in  the  dark  concerning  what  goes  on  in 
the  cerebellum,  it  may  be  worth  while  to  call  attention  once  more 
to  the  remarkable  characters  of  the  superficial  grey  matter  (§  648). 
The  many  points  of  resemblance  between  it  and  the  cerebral 
cortex  cannot  but  suggest  that  the  processes  taking  place  in  it 
have  some  analogies  with  cortical  events.  And  it  is  at  least  a 
fact  of  some  significance  that  congenital  deficiency,  or  atrophy 
of  the  cerebral  hemisphere  of  one  side,  is  frequently  accompanied 
by  a  corresponding  deficiency  of  the  crossed  cerebellar  hemisphere. 

§  688.  Both  the  anterior  and  posterior  corpora  quadri- 
gemina  are  complex  in  structure ;  not  only  do  they  differ  from 
each  other,  but  also  in  each  the  grey  matter  differs  in  different  parts, 
both  as  to  its  nature  and  appearance  and  as  to  its  connections 
with  tracts  of  fibres.  If  we  have  little  right  to  speak  of  the 
"functions  of  the  cerebellum,"  we  have  even  less  right  to  speak 
of  the  "  functions  of  the  corpora  quadrigemina  "  or  of  either  pair 
of  them.  The  anterior  pair,  as  we  have  seen,  has  to  do  in  some 
way  with  vision ;  but  we  have  reason  to  think  that  a  part  only  of 
the  whole  body  is  thus  concerned ;  and  there  is  some  foundation 
for  the  view  that  of  this  part,  one  portion  belongs,  so  to  speak,  to 
the  optic  tract  and  another  portion  to  the  cortical  fibres  of  the 
optic  radiation.  Possibly  still  another  part  is  concerned  in 
bringing,  as  we  have  (§  673)  suggested,  visual  impulses  to  bear 
on  the  coordination  of  movements. 

Stimulation   of    the   surface   of    the   posterior    pair,    besides 


CHAP.  IL]  THE   BRAIN.  *   1113 

giving  rise  to  movements  of  various  parts  of  the  body,  has  in 
monkeys  and  some  other  animals,  the  singular  effect  of  producing 
a  vocal  utterance  in  the  form  of  a  cry  or  bark.  But  we  cannot 
make  much  use  of  these  results  for  the  purpose  of  drawing 
conclusions  as  to  what  share  these  bodies  take  in  the  whole  work 
of  the  brain.  In  the  frog,  the  optic  lobes  correspond  to  the  two 
pair  of  corpora  quadrigemina  together;  and  the  cry  just  mentioned 
may  perhaps  be  put  side  by  side  with  the  fact  that  in  the  frog  the 
optic  lobes  seem  to  furnish  a  mechanism  for  croaking ;  when  the 
optic  lobes  are  destroyed,  the  reflex  croaking  mentioned  in  §  638 
is  done  away  with.  The  probable  connection  of  the  posterior 
corpora  quadrigemina  with  hearing  is  also  interesting  in  this 
connection ;  but  we  have  no  satisfactory  evidence  of  any  special 
ties  between  the  bodies  in  question  and  either  the  cortical  area  for 
phonation  or  the  vocal  mechanism  in  general;  the  occurrence  of 
the  cry  remains  so  far  an  isolated  fact. 

In  frogs,  in  which  the  cerebellum  is  very  small,  the  optic  lobes 
seem  to  be  particularly  concerned  in  the  coordination  of  movements. 
When  the  brain  is  removed  by  means  of  a  section  behind  the 
optic  lobes  the  animal  loses  the  power  of  balancing  itself  (§  638), 
which  it  possesses  when  the  section  passes  in  front  of  the  optic 
lobes ;  and  injury  to  the  optic  lobes  produces  incoordination  of 
movement  and  often  "  forced  movements."  It  has  been  maintained 
that  the  loss  of  coordination  is  in  these  cases  due  to  removal  of  or 
injury  to  the  central  grey  matter  in  the  walls  of  the  third 
ventricle,  and  not  to  mere  removal  of  or  injury  to  the  optic 
lobes ;  but  the  whole  evidence  goes  to  shew  that  in  the  frog  and 
in  the  bird  the  optic  lobes  do  play  a  part  in  the  coordination  of 
movement,  though  lesions  of  the  central  grey  matter  around  the 
third  ventricle,  or  indeed  of  the  thalamus  or  other  parts  of  the 
tegmentum,  may  give  rise  to  loss  of  coordination  or  to  "forced 
movements." 

In  the  mammal  removal  of  or  injury  to  the  posterior  corpora 
quadrigemina  does  not  cause  blindness,  but  may,  like  a  lesion  of 
the  anterior  pair,  give  rise  to  loss  of  coordination  or  to  forced 
movements;  the  effect,  however,  is  in  most  instances  very 
temporary.  The  connection  of  the  anterior  pair  with  vision 
suggests  a  clue  as  to  how  this  pair  takes  part  in  coordination ; 
but  as  to  how  the  posterior  pair  could  intervene  in  the  matter  we 
have  hardly  so  much  as  a  hint ;  for,  even  if  we  admit  a  connection 
between  them  and  the  sense  of  hearing,  and,  remembering  that 
a  loud  sound  will  often  cause  a  person  to  reel,  further  admit  that 
purely  auditory  impulses,  as  distinct  from  what  we  have  called 
ampullar  impulses,  may  take  part  in  the  general  coordination  of 
bodily  movements  and  in  the  maintenance  of  equilibrium,  as  they 
certainly  do  in  the  special  coordination  of  laryngeal  movements, 
still  we  are  not  much  nearer  an  understanding  of  the  matter. 
We  may  add  that  section  of  the  lateral  fillet,  which  appears  as  a 


iiu  SPLANCHNIC  FUNCTIONS  or  THE  BRAIN.  [BOOKIH. 

conspicuous  tie  between  the  posterior  corpora  quadrigemina  and 
the  parts  of  the  nervous  system  behind  them,  does  not  appear  to 
have  any  marked  effect  in  producing  incoordination. 

In  fine,  beyond  the  broad  facts  on  which  we  dwelt  in  a 
previous  section,  namely,  that  we  maintain  our  equilibrium  and 
carry  out  complex  movements  involving  often  several  parts  of  our 
body,  through  what  we  call  coordination,  that  afferent  impulses 
supply  important  factors  of  this  coordination,  and  that  the 
cerebellum,  through  the  vestibular  nerves  in  part  at  all  events, 
together  with  other  portions  of  the  middle  brain,  are  in  some  way 
its  chief  instruments,  we  as  yet  know  very  little.  We  have 
certainly  no  adequate  knowledge  as  to  how  either  pair  of  corpora 
quadrigemina  exactly  intervene  in  the  matter,  or,  indeed,  as  to 
what  other  parts  they  play  in  the  general  work  of  the  brain. 

With  regard  to  other  tracts  of  fibres  or  areas  of  grey  matter 
we  have  nothing  to  say,  except  as  regards  those  which  are  more 
or  less  immediately  connected  with  certain  of  the  cranial  nerves, 
such  for  instance  as  the  nerves  for  movements  of  the  eyes,  and 
these  it  will  be  best  to  consider  when  we  have  to  deal  with  the 
nerves  themselves. 

§  689.  Besides  the  somatic  functions  which  in  previous 
discussions  we  have  chiefly  had  in  view,  the  brain  as  a  whole 
undoubtedly  carries  out  splanchnic  functions;  concerning  these, 
however,  we  must  be  very  brief. 

Of  the  respiratory  and  vaso-motor  functions  of  the  bulb  we 
have  already  treated  in  their  appropriate  places,  and  we  have 
referred  (§  535)  to  the  experimental  evidence  that  a  lesion  of  the 
corpus  striatum,  or  of  the  front  part  of  the  optic  thalamus  has  a 
remarkable  influence  on  the  development  of  heat  in  the  body. 
We  have  further  seen  that  the  higher  parts  of  the  brain,  acting 
through  the  bulb,  exercise  powerful  influences  on  respiration,  on 
the  vaso-motor  system,  and  on  the  beat  of  the  heart.  Daily 
experience  affords  abundant  instances  of  actions  such  as  these, 
as  well  as  of  the  influence  of  the  brain  on  other  organic  functions. 
We  can  bring  our  will  to  bear  on  the  mechanism  of  micturition 
(§  430)  which  is  almost  wholly,  and  on  the  mechanism  of 
defaecation  (§  275)  which  is  largely,  splanchnic  in  nature.  These 
movements,  however,  are  not  skilled  movements ;  and  as  we 
explained  in  dealing  with  them,  the  action  of  the  brain  as  regards 
them  seems  limited  to  augmenting  or  inhibiting  the  activity  of 
spinal  centres.  We  should  therefore  hardly  expect  them  to  be 
specially  represented  in  the  cortical  motor  region.  But  emotions 
have  a  much  wider  and  more  powerful  influence  over  the  splanchnic 
functions  than  has  the  will,  and  have  the  power  of  affecting  the 
work  of  certain  organs,  for  instance  the  heart  and  secreting  glands, 
which  the  will  is  unable  to  touch.  And  since  we  have  every 
reason  to  believe  that  the  cortex  is  closely  associated  with  the 
emotions,  we  may  naturally  infer  that  elements  of  the  cortex 


CHAP,  ii.]  THE   BRAIN.  1115 

supply  a  link  in  the  chain  through  which  an  emotion  influences 
this  or  that  splanchnic  activity;  we  may,  accordingly,  expect  to 
find  that  stimulation  of  some  part  or  other  of  the  cortex  produces 
splanchnic  effects.  The  results  of  experimental  investigation, 
however,  are  both  scanty  and  discordant ;  but  the  greater  weight 
should  perhaps  be  attached  to  the  positive  results.  Thus,  some 
observers  find  that  stimulation  of  the  cortex,  the  locality  being  in 
the  dog  some  part  of  the  sigmoid  gyrus,  produces  movements  of 
the  bladder ;  and  they  trace  the  path  of  this  influence  through  the 
front  part  of  the  thalamus  and  the  tegmentum  to  the  bulb  and  so 
to  the  cord,  excluding  the  cerebellum,  which  other  observers 
believed  to  be  concerned  in  the  matter.  Some  observers  again  find 
that  stimulation  of  the  cortex  produces  a  flow  of  '  chorda  saliva,' 
while  others  maintain  that  the  secretion,  when  it  does  occur,  is  an 
indirect  and  not  a  direct  effect  of  the  cortical  stimulation ;  and  it 
may  be  remarked  that  the  cortical  area,  which  is  claimed  to  be  a 
"salivation  area,"  lying  in  the  dog  on  the  convolutions  dorsal  to 
and  in  front  of  the  Sylvian  fissure,  is  not  either  the  area  connected 
with  the  facial  nerve,  or  that  allotted  to  taste  or  smell. 

Similarly,  stimulation  of  parts  of  the  cortex  has  in  the  hands 
of  various  observers  led  to  movements  or  to  arrest  of  movements 
of  the  intestines,  to  changes  in  the  beat  of  the  heart,  and  to 
various  vaso-motor  and  other  effects ;  but  it  will  not  be  profitable 
to  enter  into  any  further  details.  We  may,  however,  add  the 
remark  that  when  the  cortical  motor  area  for  a  limb  is  removed, 
or  suffers  a  lesion,  the  temporary  paralysis  which  is  thereby  caused 
is  accompanied  by  a  rise  of  temperature  in  the  limb;  this  may 
be  at  times  very  great  indeed ;  in  the  monkey  for  instance,  the 
hand  or  foot  on  the  paralysed  side  may  be  as  much  as  5°  C. 
higher  than  that  of  the  other  side.  The  effect  is  partly  due 
to  vaso-motor  paralysis,  but,  especially  considering  that  the 
muscles  of  the  limb  are  relatively  quiescent  and  so  producing  less 
heat  than  usual,  cannot  be  due  to  that  alone.  The  remarkable 
result  may  be  taken  as  still  further  illustrating  the  complexity  of 
the  processes  connected  with  the  cortical  motor  area ;  the  area  is 
in  some  way  associated  with  the  vascular  arrangements  and 
nutrition  of  the  muscles  with  whose  movements  it  is  concerned. 

§  690.  There  remain  yet  a  few  words  to  be  said  about  the 
cortex.  We  regard,  and  justly  so,  the  spontaneous  intrinsic 
activity  of  the  brain  as  the  most  striking  feature  of  its  life.  The 
nearest  approach  to  it  which  we  find  elsewhere  in  the  body,  is 
perhaps  the  rhythmic  beat  of  the  heart.  The  analogy  between  the 
"regular  automatism"  of  the  one,  and  the  "  irregular  automatism" 
of  the  other  is  a  striking  one ;  and  indeed  our  knowledge  of  the 
relatively  simple  spontaneity  of  the  heart  has  probably  influenced 
to  a  large  extent  our  conceptions  of  the  complex  spontaneity  of 
the  brain.  In  the  heart  the  rhythmic  discharge  of  energy  is 
chiefly  determined  by  intrinsic  chemical  changes,  by  the  meta- 


1116  THE   CORTEX.  [BOOK  in. 

bolism  of  the  cardiac  substance ;  the  influence  of  external 
circumstances,  apart  from  those  which  provide  an  adequate  supply 
of  proper  blood,  is  wholly  subsidiary  and  serves  only  to  raise  or 
to  lower  the  intrinsic  changes  from  time  to  time,  as  occasion  may 
demand.  And  the  analogy  of  the  heart  has  perhaps  led  us  to 
exaggerate  the  part  played  in  the  brain  by  the  like  intrinsic 
chemical  metabolism.  (We  are  here  of  course  viewing  the  action 
of  the  brain  from  the  only  stand-point  admissible  in  these  pages, 
the  purely  physiological  one ;  but  such  a  mode  of  treatment  does 
not  prejudge  other  points  of  view.)  Some  writers  use  expressions 
which  seem  to  imply  the  conception  that  the  nervous  changes 
forming  the  basis  of  the  psychical  and  other  processes  of  the  brain 
are  chiefly  the  direct  outcome  of  the  chemical  metabolism  of  the 
grey  matter  and  especially  of  the  nerve  cells.  They  speak  of  "  the 
discharge  of  energy"  from  these  cells  in  the  same  way  that  we 
can  speak  of  the  discharge  of  energy  from  a  cardiac  fibre.  But,  to 
say  nothing  of  the  low  rate  of  nervous  metabolism  as  measured 
in  terms  of  chemical  energy,  we  have  no  experimental  or  other 
evidence  of  nervous  substance  in  any  part  of  the  body  being,  like 
the  cardiac  substance,  the  seat  of  an  important  metabolism 
carried  on  irrespective  of  influences  other  than  purely  nutritive 
ones.  In  the  case  of  nerve  cells  interpolated  along  nerves 
composed  of  fibres  of  the  same  kind,  as  in  the  sporadic  ganglia, 
all  the  instances  where  the  nerve  cells  were  supposed  to  initiate 
active  processes  have,  on  examination,  broken  down ;  as  we  have 
seen,  the  ganglia  of  the  heart  do  not  supply  the  moving  cause  of 
the  heart  beat.  It  is  only  in  the  central  nervous  system  where  nerve- 
cells,  as  part  of  grey  matter,  are  found  at  the  meeting  of  nerve- 
fibres  of  different  kinds,  that  we  have  any  evidence  of  "  discharge 
of  energy"  from  the  cells. 

As  we  pointed  out  (§597)  in  speaking  of  the  spinal  cord,  the 
discharge  of  efferent  impulses  from  the  central  nervous  system, 
though  it  undoubtedly  must  have  a  certain  chemical  basis,  namely, 
the  metabolism  of  the  nervous  substance,  is,  in  the  first  line, 
dependent  on  the  advent  of  afferent  impulses.  But  this,  if  true  of 
the  spinal  cord,  is  still  more  true  of  the  brain,  which  receives  or 
may  receive  not  only  all  the  impulses  which  reach  it  through  the 
cord,  but  especially  potent  and  varied  impulses  directly  through 
the  cranial  nerves.  All  life  long  the  never  ceasing  changes  of 
the  external  world  continually  break  as  waves  on  the  peripheral 
endings  of  the  afferent  nerves,  all  lifelong  nervous  impulses,  now 
more  now  fewer,  are  continually  sweeping  inwards  towards  the 
centre ;  and  the  nervous  metabolism,  which  is  the  basis  of  nervous 
action,  must  be  at  least  as  largely  dependent  on  these  influences 
from  without,  as  on  the  mere  chemical  supply  furnished  by  the 
blood. 

We  have  developed  this  point  because  of  the  influence  it 
must  have  on  our  conceptions  of  the  physiological  processes  taking 


CHAP.  IL]  THE   BRAIN.  1117 

place  in  the  cortex.  If  we  accept  the  view  just  laid  down, 
we  must  regard  the  supereminent  activity  of  the  cortex  and 
the  characters  of  the  processes  taking  place  in  it  as  due  not  so 
much  to  the  intrinsic  chemical  nature  of  the  nervous  substance 
which  is  built  up  into  the  cortical  grey  matter  as  to  the  fact  that 
impulses  are  continually  streaming  into  it  from  all  parts  of  the 
body,  that  almost  all  influences  brought  to  bear  on  the  body 
make  themselves  felt  by  it.  To  put  the  matter  in  a  bald  way 
we  may  ask  the  question,  what  would  happen  in  the  cortex  if,  its 
ordinary  nutritive  supply  remaining  as  before,  it  were  cut  adrift 
from  afferent  impulses  of  all  kinds  ?  We  can  hardly  doubt  but 
that  volitional  and  other  psychical  processes  would  soon  come  to 
a  standstill  and  consciousness  vanish.  This  is  indeed  roughly 
indicated  by  the  remarkable  case  of  a  patient,  whose  almost  only 
communication  with  the  external  world  was  by  means  of  one  eye, 
he  being  blind  of  the  other  eye,  deaf  of  both  ears,  and  suffering 
from  general  anesthesia.  Whenever  the  sound  eye  was  closed,  he 
went  to  sleep.  It  is  further  indirectly  illustrated  by  the  following 
experimental  result.  We  have  seen  (§  654)  that  a  vertical  incision 
carried  through  the  depth  of  the  grey  matter  around  an  area  does 
not  prevent  stimulation  of  the  surface  of  the  area  producing  the 
usual  movements.  But  after  such  an  incision  the  animal  suffers  a 
paralysis  of  the  movements  connected  with  the  area,  like  that 
resulting  from  the  removal  of  the  grey  matter  of  the  area ;  and 
the  operation  is  said  to  be  followed  by  degenerative  changes  in 
the  area,  and  degeneration  of  the  pyramidal  fibres  starting  from  it. 
Some  of  this  effect  may  be  due  to  nutritive  changes  brought  about 
by  injury  to  the  pia  mater  and  division  of  blood  vessels;  but  it 
cannot  be  wholly  accounted  for  in  this  way ;  it  appears  as  if  the 
life  of  the  area  is  curtailed  when  its  nervous  ties  are  broken. 

We  may  conclude  then  that  we  are  not  justified  in  speaking 
of  consciousness  or  volition,  or  other  psychical  processes,  even 
admitting  that  these  fail  when  the  cortex  is  removed,  as  being 
functions  of  the  cortex  in  the  same  way  that  we  speak  of  the 
functions  of  other  organs ;  they  are  rather  functions  of  the  con- 
nections of  the  cortex  with  the  other  parts  of  the  central  nervous 
system. 

We  should  add  that  they  are  also  functions  of  the  connections 
of  the  several  parts  of  the  cortex  with  each  other.  All  our 
knowledge  goes  to  shew  that  psychical  processes  are  dependent 
on,  or  are  in  some  way  associated  with  the  cortex ;  but  whatever 
classification  of  psychical  functions  we  adopt,  we  are  wholly  unable 
to  make  out  any  localisation  of  functions,  such  as  we  can  make 
out  for  movements,  visual  sensations  and  the  like.  Even  taking 
the  broad  and  elementary  division  into  "  the  emotions "  and  "  the 
intellect,"  we  cannot  satisfactorily  allot  either  division  to  any 
particular  part  of  the  hemisphere.  In  dogs,  removal  of  particular 
parts  of  the  hemispheres  has  indeed  been  observed  to  change  the 


1118     PSYCHICAL  PROCESSES  IN  THE  CORTEX.    [Boon  m. 

character  of  the  animal,  converting  for  instance  a  vicious,  morose 
dog  into  a  mild  and  inoffensive  one ;  and  removal  of  the  front 
parts  of  the  hemisphere  seems  to  have  frequently  a  marked 
effect  in  rendering  the  animal  more  impressionable  and  excitable ; 
he  becomes  much  more  demonstrative  and  'gushing'  in  his 
behaviour  than  before.  But  these  are  mere  hints,  and  the 
clinical  histories  of  disease  in  man  do  not  enable  us  to  say 
much  more.  Such  knowledge  as  we  do  possess  rather  .tends  to 
shew  that  the  psychical  processes  in  proportion  as  they  become 
more  complex  involve  a  greater  number  of  nervous  factors,  and 
therefore  have  for  their  material  basis  a  greater  width  of  nervous 
area,  or  in  other  words  their  localisation  becomes  less  definite. 
Thus  while  we  may  localize  the  beginning  of  a  psychical  process, 
a  visual  sensation  for  instance,  and  one  of  its  terminal  acts  such 
as  the  issue  of  impulses  along  the  pyramidal  tract,  we  cannot  put 
our  finger  on  the  seat  of  the  intermediate  transactions.  These 
even  in  the  simplest  processes  must  be  complex,  and  must  involve 
many  factors.  Our  simplest  conceptions  of  the  external  world  are 
based  on  a  combination  of  visual  sensations  and  tactile  sensations. 
It  being  granted  that  the  visual  sensation,  in  one  phase  of  its 
development,  is  connected  with  certain  changes  in  some  spot  of 
the  occipital  cortex,  there  must  be  some  tie  between  this  and 
the  corresponding  nervous  seat  of  the  tactile  sensation  wherever 
that  may  be,  and  further  ties  between  these  and  other  parts  of 
the  cortex.  Hence  as  we  said  the  psychical  process  is  a  function 
of  connections. 

Many  of  these  ties  are  most  probably  furnished  by  the 
association  fibres  passing  from  one  part  of  the  cortex  to  a 
neighbouring  part.  We  must  also  probably  admit  that  impulses 
or  to  use  a  more  general  word,  processes,  may  travel  laterally 
along  the  tangle  of  the  cortical  grey  matter,  for  this,  like  the 
grey  matter  of  the  spinal  cord,  seems  to  form  a  physiological 
continuity,  no  more  broken  by  the  fissures  than  is  the  cord  by 
its  segmental  arrangement;  and  we  know  nothing  as  to  the 
limits  which  must  be  placed  on  the  distance  to  which  such 
processes  may  travel  from  their  focus  of  origin.  Further,  seeing 
how  completely  in  the  dark  we  are  as  to  the  reason  why  we 
possess  two  hemispheres,  and  especially  seeing  that,  as  shewn 
by  speech,  the  whole  of  each  hemisphere  is  not  identical  in 
action  with  the  whole  of  the  other,  we  may  perhaps  suppose 
that  the  fibres  of  the  corpus  callosum,  which  form  so  large  a 
part  of  the  central  white  matter  of  the  hemisphere,  have  other 
duties  than  that  of  merely  keeping  the  points  of  one  hemisphere 
in  touch  with  the  corresponding  points  of  the  other  hemisphere. 
But,  when  we  have  made  every  allowance  for  all  these  direct 
intercortical  connections,  we  are  driven  to  the  conclusion  that 
the  indirect  ties  between  one  part  of  the  cortex  and  another 
through  the  lower  parts  of  the  brain  are  of  no  less,  perhaps  of 


CHAP,  ii.]  THE   BRAIN.  1119 

greater  importance.  This  indeed  is  shewn  by  the  relations  of 
the  motor  region.  We  have  already  urged,  that  even  as  regards 
the  mere  carrying  out  of  a  skilled  movement  (and  we  may 
add  whether  that  be  voluntary  or  involuntary  in  the  ordinary, 
common  use  of  the  words)  the  motor  region  must  have  other 
ties  with  the  part  moved  than  merely  the  efferent  tie  of  the 
pyramidal  fibres;  it  must  have  sensory  afferent  ties,  and  the 
course  of  these,  including  even  perhaps  those  which  belong  to 
the  muscular  sense,  we  may  regard  as  an  indirect  one  along 
the  spinal  cord  and  middle  parts  of  the  brain,  though  the 
details  are  as  yet  unknown  to  us.  It  must  moreover,  as  we 
have  also  seen,  have  ties,  at  least  in  many  cases,  with  parts  other 
than  the  part  moved,  for  instance  with  the  general  coordinating 
machinery.  And  the  ease  with  which  some,  not  very  obvious, 
change,  will  permit  the  stimulation  of  a  limited  motor  area  to 
start  epileptiform  convulsions,  shews  how  many  and  close  are  the 
ties  in  another  direction.  Further,  when  we  go  beyond  the  final 
phases  of  the  process  in  the  motor  cortex,  to  those  which  precede 
the  issue  of  the  efferent  impulses,  we  find  the  ties  multiplying. 
For  instance,  since  our  movements  are  so  largely  guided  by  visual 
sensations,  there  must  be  ties  between  the  motor  cortex  and 
the  central  visual  apparatus,  it  may  be  of  the  occipital  cortex, 
but  it  may  also  be  of  the  lower  visual  centres.  As  we  insisted, 
the  motor  area  is  only  a  link  in  a  complex  chain  ;  and  what 
we  can  see,  dimly  though  it  be,  in  reference  to  the  cortical 
motor  processes,  probably  holds  good  for  those  other  cortical 
processes  as  well,  of  whose  nervous  genesis  we  know  at  present 
nothing.  Hence  even  the  higher  psychical  events  cannot  truly 
be  spoken  of  as  functions  of  the  cortex,  meaning  that  they  are 
simply  the  outcome  of  molecular  changes  in  the  cortical  grey 
matter;  they  are  rather  to  be  regarded  as  the  outcome  of 
complex  processes  in  which  the  parts  of  the  brain  below  the 
cortex  play  a  part  no  less  important  than  that  of  the  cortex 
itself.  If  so,  the  fibres  passing  down  from  the  cortex  to  the 
middle  brain  have  functions  by  which  they  take  part  even  in 
our  psychical  life,  functions  for  which  neither  the  words  motor 
nor  sensory  are  fitting. 


P.  71 


SEC.    11.     ON   THE   TIME   TAKEN   UP   BY   CEREBRAL 
OPERATIONS. 

§  691.  We  have  already  seen  (§  594)  that  a  considerable  time 
is  taken  up  in  a  purely  reflex  act,  such  as  that  of  winking,  though 
this  is  perhaps  the  most  rapid  form  of  reflex  movement.  When 
the  movement  which  is  executed  in  response  to  a  stimulus  involves 
cerebral  operations  a  still  longer  time  is  needed ;  and  the  interval 
between  the  application  of  the  stimulus  and  the  commencement 
of  the  muscular  contraction  varies  according  to  the  nature  of  the 
mental  labour  involved. 

The  simplest  case  is  that  in  which  a  person  makes  a  signal 
immediately  that  he  perceives  a  stimulus,  ex.  gr.  closes  or  opens  a 
galvanic  circuit  the  moment  that  he  feels  an  induction  shock 
applied  to  the  skin,  or  sees  a  flash  of  light,  or  hears  a  sound.  By 
arrangements  similar  to  those  employed  in  measuring  the  velocity 
of  nervous  impulses,  the  moment  of  the  application  of  the  stimulus 
and  the  moment  of  the  making  of  the  signal  are  both  recorded 
on  the  same  travelling  surface,  and  the  interval  between  them 
is  carefully  measured.  This  interval,  which  has  been  called  '  the 
reaction  period'  or  'reaction  time/  may  be  divided  into  three 
stages :  (1)  The  time  during  which  afferent  impulses  are  generated 
in  the  peripheral  sense  organs  and  transmitted  along  the  afferent 
nerves  to  the  central  nervous  system ;  this  may  be  called  the 
"  afferent  stage."  (2)  The  time  during  which,  through  the  opera- 
tions of  the  central  nervous  system,  the  afferent  impulses  are 
transformed  into  efferent  impulses;  this  may  be  called  the  "central 
stage."  (3)  The  time  taken  up  by  the  passage  of  the  efferent 
impulses  along  the  efferent  nerves  and  the  transformation  of  the 
nervous  impulses  into  muscular  contractions;  this  may  be  called 
the  "efferent  stage."  In  the  efferent  stage  the  events  are  com- 
paratively simple,  and  though  not  absolutely  constant,  do  not 
vary  largely ;  we  are  able  to  form  a  fairly  satisfactory  estimate  of 
its  duration  and  so  of  the  share  in  the  whole  reaction  period  which 
may  be  allotted  to  it.  The  events  of  the  afferent  stage  are 
much  more  complex  and  the  estimates  of  its  duration,  being 
arrived  at  in  an  indirect  manner,  and  chiefly  based  upon  calcu- 
lations of  the  whole  reaction  time,  are  very  uncertain.  Hence  all 


CHAP.  IL]  THE   BRAIN.  1121 

attempts  to  estimate  the  length  of  the  "  central "  stage,  the 
"reduced  reaction  period"  as  it  is  sometimes  called,  by  subtracting 
the  efferent  and  afferent  stages,  must  be  subject  to  much  error. 
But  a  good  deal  may  be  learnt  by  studying  the  variations  under 
different  circumstances  of  the  reaction  period  as  a  whole. 

Taking  first  of  all  the  cases  in  which  the  events  of  the  central 
stage  are  simple,  such  as  those  where  the  subject  has  merely  to 
make  a  signal  upon  feeling  a  sensation,  we  find  that  the  length 
of  the  reaction  period  is  dependent  on  the  intensity  of  the 
stimulus,  being  shorter  with  the  stronger  stimulus.  But  varia- 
tions in  the  strength  of  the  stimulus,  especially  in  the  case  of 
minimal  stimuli,  have  a  much  more  striking  effect  in  determining 
the  certainty  of  the  reaction  than  in  affecting  the  length  of  the 
period.  Thus  when  the  signal  is  made  in  response  to  some  visual 
sensation,  upon  seeing  an  electric  spark  for  instance,  if  the  spark 
be  a  very  weak  one  the  subject  of  the  experiment  often  fails  to 
make  the  signal  at  all,  though  he  may  rarely  fail  if  the  spark  be  a 
strong  one. 

Some  of  the  most  marked  variations  in  the  length  of  the 
reaction  period  are  determined  by  the  individuality  of  the  subject. 
Thus  with  the  same  stimulus  applied  under  the  same  circum- 
stances the  reaction  period  of  one  person  will  be  found  very 
different  from  that  of  another. 

The  length  of  the  reaction  period  varies  also  according  to  the 
nature  and  disposition  of  the  peripheral  organs  stimulated.  In 
general  it  may  be  said  that  cutaneous  sensations  produced  by 
the  stimulus  of  an  electric  shock  applied  to  the  skin  (the  signal 
for  instance  being  made  by  the  right  hand  when  the  shock  is  felt 
by  the  left  hand)  are  followed  by  a  shorter  reaction  period  than 
are  auditory  sensations,  while  the  period  of  these  is  in  turn 
shorter  than  that  of  visual  sensations  produced  by  luminous 
objects;  on  the  other  hand,  the  shortest  period  of  all  is  said 
to  be  that  of  visual  sensations  produced  by  direct  electrical 
stimulation  of  the  retina.  Roughly  speaking  we  may  say  that 
the  reaction  period  is  for  cutaneous  sensations  fth,  for  hearing 
£th,  and  for  sight  -^th  of  a  second. 

Practice  materially  shortens  the  reaction  period ;  indeed,  after 
long  practice,  making  the  signal,  at  first  a  distinct  effort  of  the 
will,  takes  on  the  characters  of  a  reflex  act,  with  a  correspond- 
ingly shortened  interval.  Lastly,  we  may  add  that  in  the  same 
individual  and  with  the  same  stimulus,  the  length  of  the  period 
will  vary  according  to  circumstances,  such  as  the  time  of  year, 
the  weather,  and  the  like,  as  well  as  according  to  the  condition  of 
the  individual,  whether  fresh  or  fatigued,  fasting  or  replete,  having 
taken  more  or  less  alcohol,  and  the  like. 

The  reaction  period  of  vision  has  long  been  known  to  astrono- 
mers. It  was  early  found  that  when  two  observers  were  watching 
the  appearance  of  the  same  star,  a  considerable  discrepancy  existed 

71—2 


1122      DURATION   OF   PSYCHICAL  PROCESSES.     [BOOK  in. 

between  their  respective  reaction  periods,  and  that  the  difference, 
forming  the  basis  of  the  so-called  '  personal  equation/  varied  from 
time  to  time  according  to  the  personal  conditions  of  the  observers. 

§  692.  The  events  taking  place  in  the  central  stage  are  of 
course  complex,  and  this  stage  may  be  subdivided  into  several 
stages.  Without  attempting  to  enter  into  psychological  questions, 
we  may  at  least  recognize  certain  elementary  distinctions.  The 
afferent  impulses  started  by  the  stimulus,  whatever  be  their 
nature,  when  they  reach  the  central  nervous  system  undergo 
changes,  and  as  we  have  seen,  probably  complex  changes  before 
they  become  sensations ;  and  further  changes,  now  of  a  more 
distinctly  psychical  character,  are  necessary  before  the  mind  can 
duly  appreciate  the  characters  of  these  sensations  and  act  accord- 
ingly. Then  come  the  psychical  processes  through  which  these 
appreciated  sensations,  or  perceptions,  or  apperceptions  as  they  are 
sometimes  called,  determine  an  act  of  volition.  Lastly,  there  are 
the  executive  processes  of  volition,  the  processes  which,  psychical 
to  begin  with,  end  in  the  issue  of  coordinate  motor  impulses,  or, 
in  other  words,  start  the  distinctly  physiological  processes  of  the 
efferent  stage.  We  may  thus  speak  of  the  time  required  for  the 
perception  of  the  stimulation,  of  the  time  required  for  the  action 
of  the  will,  and  of  the  time  required  for  the  complex  psychical 
processes  which  link  these  two  together.  Accepting  this  elemen- 
tary analysis,  it  is  obvious  that  the  total  length  of  the  central 
stage  may  be  varied  by  differences  in  the  length  of  each  of  these 
parts ;  and  a  more  complete  analysis  would  of  course  open  the 
way  for  farther  distinctions.  Hence,  by  studying  the  variations 
of  the  whole  reaction  time  under  varying  forms  of  psychical 
activity,  we  may  form  an  estimate  of  time  taken  up  by  various 
psychical  processes. 

We  may  take  as  an  instance  the  case  in  which  the  subject 
of  the  experiment  has  to  exercise  discrimination.  The  mode  of 
making  the  signal  being  the  same,  and  the  stimulus  being  of 
the  same  order  in  each  trial,  that  is  to  say,  visual,  or  cutaneous, 
or  auditory,  &c.,  and  general  circumstances  remaining  the  same, 
two  different  stimuli  are  employed,  and  the  subject  is  required 
to  make  a  signal  in  response  to  the  one  stimulus,  but  not  to  the 
other;  the  subject  has  to  discriminate  between  the  psychical 
effects  of  the  two  stimuli.  Suppose,  for  example,  the  stimulus 
is  the  sound  of  a  spoken  or  sung  vowel,  and  the  subject  is 
required  to  make  a  signal  when  a  is  spoken  or  sung,  but  not 
when  o  is  spoken  or  sung.  If  the  subject's  whole  reaction  period 
be  determined  (i)  in  the  usual  way,  with  either  a  or  o  spoken  (and 
the  result  will  be  found  not  to  differ  materially  whether  a  or  o  be 
used),  the  subject  knowing  that  only  a  or  only  o  will  be  spoken, 
and  then  be  determined  again  (ii)  when  he  has  to  discriminate 
in  order  that  he  may  make  the  signal  when  a  is  spoken  but  not 
when  o  is  spoken,  he  not  knowing  which  is  about  to  be  spoken, 


CHAP,  ii.]  THE   BRAIN.  1123 

the  whole  reaction  period  will  be  found  to  be  distinctly  longer  in 
the  second  case.  The  experiment  may  be  varied  by  making  use 
of  all  the  vowel  sounds  taken  irregularly  as  the  stimulus,  the 
subject  responding  by  a  signal  to  one  only,  as  arranged  beforehand. 
And  of  course  other  orders  of  stimulus  may  be  used,  either  visual, 
the  signal  being  made  when  a  red  light  is  shewn  but  not  when 
other  colours  are  shewn,  or  tactile,  the  signal  being  made  when 
one  part  of  the  body  is  touched  but  not  when  other  parts  are 
touched,  and  the  like. 

In  such  experiments  where  the  subject  has  to  distinguish,  to 
discriminate  between  two  or  more  events,  the  prolongation  of  the 
reaction  period  is  obviously  due  to  the  longer  time  required  for 
the  psychical  processes  taking  place  during  what  we  have  called 
the  central  stage.  In  the  two  cases,  one  without  and  the  other 
with  discrimination,  not  only  are  the  afferent  and  efferent  stages 
the  same  in  both,  but  we  have  no  reason  to  suppose  that  in  the 
central  stage  is  there  any  difference  between  the  two  cases  as 
to  the  time  taken  up  by  the  transformation  of  simple  sensory 
impulses  into  perceptions,  or  as  to  that  taken  up  by  the  will 
in  gaining  access  to  the  motor  apparatus  and  so  starting  the 
processes  of  the  efferent  stage ;  the  delay  takes  place  in  the 
psychical  processes  intervening  between  these  two  parts,  and  the 
amount  of  delay  is  the  measure  of  the  time  needed  for  the 
processes  involved  in  the  discrimination.  This  "discrimination 
period"  has  been  found  to  differ  in  the  same  individual 
according  to  the  sensation  employed,  visual,  auditory,  &c.,  and 
according  to  the  kind  of  difference  in  the  sensation  which  has 
to  be  discriminated,  for  instance  in  visual  sensations  between 
colours  or  between  objects  in  different  parts  of  the  field  of  vision. 
In  a  series  of  observations  made  in  this  way,  the  discrimination 
period,  i.e.  the  prolongation  of  the  simple  reaction  period  due  to 
having  to  discriminate,  was  found  to  range  from  0*011  sec.  to 
0-062  sec. 

Another  series  of  observations  may  be  made  in  the  following 
way.  The  signal  being  one  made  with  the  hand,  the  simple 
reaction  period  for  a  stimulus  is  determined  with  the  signal  given 
by  the  right  hand.  Two  kinds  of  stimuli  are  then  employed,  both 
of  the  same  order,  two  vowel  sounds  for  instance,  and  the  subject 
is  directed  to  respond  to  one  vowel  with  the  right  hand  and  to 
the  other  with  the  left  hand.  It  is  found,  the  subject  being  right- 
handed,  that  the  reaction  period  is  greater  when  the  signal  is  made 
with  the  left  hand.  In  this  case  the  delay  takes  place  not  in  the 
recognition  of  the  effects  of  the  stimulus,  nor  in  the  processes 
through  which  the  will  is  formed  upon  that  recognition ;  these  are 
the  same  in  the  two  cases;  it  takes  place  in  the  processes  by 
which  the  will  is  brought  to  bear  on  the  nervous  motor  apparatus 
for  making  the  signal,  on  the  cortical  origin,  for  example  of  the 
pyramidal  tract ;  these  processes  take  a  longer  time  in  the  case  of 


1124      DURATION   OF   PSYCHICAL   PROCESSES.     [BOOK  in. 

the  unaccustomed  left  hand  than  in  the  case  of  the  usual  right 
hand.  In  this  way  we  obtain  a  measure  of  so  to  speak  the 
volitional  side  of  psychical  processes. 

In  a  somewhat  similar  way  we  may  obtain  a  measure  of  the 
time  required  for  perception.  A  strong  sensation  following  too 
closely  upon  a  weak  one  will  prevent  the  psychical  recognition  of 
the  weaker  one.  If,  for  instance,  two  or  three  letters  in  white  on 
a  black  background  be  presented  to  the  eye,  and  a  large  white 
surface  be  presented  afterwards  at  an  interval  which  is  made 
successively  shorter  and  shorter,  it  is  found  that  when  the  interval 
is  made  very  brief  indeed  the  letters  cannot  be  perceived  at  all. 
In  proportion  as  the  interval  is  prolonged,  the  recognition  of  the 
letters  increases,  until  at  an  interval  of  about  '05  sec.  they  are 
fully  and  clearly  recognized.  That  is  to  say,  the  time  required  for 
perception  is  in  such  a  case  of  about  that  length. 

The  duration  of  all  these  psychical  processes,  as  of  the  simple 
reaction  period  itself,  varies  of  course  under  different  circum- 
stances, and  the  discrimination  period  may  be  conveniently  used 
for  measurements  of  the  varying  effects  of  circumstances.  Practice 
shortens  the  discrimination  period  as  it  does  the  simple  reaction 
period.  One  of  the  most  powerful  influences  is  that  of  attention. 
And  it  is  stated  that  the  shortening  of  the  period  is  greater  when 
the  attention  is  concentrated  on  the  making  of  the  signal  than 
when  it  is  more  especially  directed  to  recognition  of  the  stimulus ; 
in  other  words,  the  volitional  processes  are  more  amenable  than 
are  the  perceptive  processes  to  the  psychical  action  which  we  call 
attention.  On  the  other  hand,  the  period  is  distinctly  prolonged 
if  the  observer  be  distracted  by  concomitant  sensations.  For 
example,  the  period  for  discriminating  between  two  visual  sen- 
sations is  prolonged  if  powerful  auditory  sensations  be  excited  at 
the  same  time. 

The  same  method  of  measurement  may  be  used  in  other  ways 
and  under  other  circumstances  with  reference  to  psychical  pro- 
cesses. It  must  be  remembered,  however,  that  all  such  obser- 
vations are  open  to  many  fallacies  and  need  particular  caution. 
It  not  unfrequently  happens  that  false  results  are  obtained;  for 
instance,  the  subject,  expecting  the  stimulus  to  be  brought  to  bear 
upon  him  and  straining  his  attention,  makes  the  signal  before  the 
stimulus  actually  comes  off.  And  the  interpretation  of  the  results 
obtained  are  in  many  cases  very  difficult ;  but  it  would  be  out  of 
place  to  dwell  upon  these  matters  any  further  here. 


SEC.    12.     THE   LYMPHATIC   ARRANGEMENTS   OF 
THE   BRAIN   AND   SPINAL   CORD. 


§  693.  The  Membranes  of  the  Brain  and  Spinal  Cord.  The 
cerebro-spinal  canal  is  lined  by  a  tough  lamellated  membrane, 
composed  of  connective  tissue  with  a  small  amount  of  elastic 
networks,  called  the  dura  mater,  which,  somewhat  closely  adherent 
to  the  walls  of  the  cranial  cavity,  is  separated  from  those  of  the 
vertebral  canal  by  a  considerable  space,  containing  blood  vessels, 
especially  large  venous  sinuses,  and  some  fat.  It  may  be 
considered  as  a  development  of  the  periosteum  lining  the 
cerebro-spinal  cavity.  It  sends  tubular  sheaths  for  some  distance 
along  the  several  cranial  and  spinal  nerves;  and  forms  between 
the  cerebral  hemispheres,  in  the  longitudinal  fissure,  a  conspicuous 
sickle-shaped  vertical  fold,  the  falx  cerebri,  as  well  as  a  smaller 
horizontal  or  oblique  fold  between  the  cerebellum  and  cerebrum 
known  as  the  tentorium. 

The  vascular  pia  mater  is  closely  attached  to  the  surface  of 
the  brain  and  spinal  cord,  dipping  down  as  we  have  seen  into  the 
ventral  or  anterior  fissure  of  the  cord  as  well  as  into  the  fissures 
of  the  brain.  Sheath-like  investments  of  pia  mater  are  continued 
along  the  several  nerves  as  they  leave  the  cerebro-spinal  cavity ; 
and  in  the  vertebral  canal  an  imperfect  partition  half-way  between 
the  dorsal  and  ventral  surfaces  of  the  cord  is  furnished  by  a 
membrane  of  connective  tissue  which,  continuous  along  its  whole 
length  with  the  pia  mater,  is  attached  to  and  fused  with  the  dura 
mater  at  intervals  only,  namely,  between  the  successive  nerve 
roots.  Since  its  outer  edge  has  thus  a  toothed  appearance,  this 
membrane  is  called  the  ligamentum  denticulatum.  Between  the 
pia  mater  next  to  the  brain  and  cord  and  the  dura  mater  next 
to  the  bony  walls  is  a  cavity,  which  is  divided  into  two  by  a 
thin  membrane,  the  arachnoid,  composed  of  interwoven  bundles 
of  connective  tissue.  The  space  between  the  arachnoid  and  the 
dura  mater  is  called  the  subdural  space,  and  the  space  between 
the  arachnoid  and  the  pia  mater  is  called  the  subarachnoid  space. 
When  the  brain  is  exposed  by  removing  the  roof  of  the  skull  and 
slitting  open  the  dura  mater,  the  subdural  space  is  laid  bare,  and 
the  arachnoid  is  seen  stretching  over  the  pia  mater;  in  the 


1126  THE   MEMBRANES.  [BOOK  m. 

vertebral  canal  the  arachnoid  lies  close  to  the  dura  mater,  so  that 
usually,  when  the  dura  mater  is  slit  open  and  turned  back,  the 
arachnoid  is  carried  with  it  and  the  cavity  exposed  is  that  of  the 
subarachnoid  space.  The  arachnoid,  like  the  dura  mater  and  the 
pia  mater,  is  continued  for  some  distance  over  the  nerves  as  they 
leave  the  cerebro-spinal  cavity;  so  that  each  nerve  at  its  exit  is 
surrounded  by  a  tubular  prolongation  of  the  subdural  space,  and 
within  this  a  similar  tubular  prolongation  of  the  subarachnoid 
space. 

The  subdural  space  is  broken  up  to  a  slight  extent  only 
by  bridles  carrying  nerves  and  blood  vessels,  especially  venous 
sinuses,  between  the  pia  mater  and  dura  mater,  and,  over  the 
surface  of  the  brain,  by  villus-like  projections  of  the  arachnoid, 
called  Pacchionian  glands,  some  of  which  pierce  the  venous 
sinuses  of  the  dura  mater.  It  is  lined  throughout,  both  on  its 
dural  and  on  its  arachnoid  wall,  by  an  epithelium  of  flat  epi- 
thelioid  cells,  and  may  be  compared  to  a  serous  cavity  such  as 
that  of  the  peritoneum.  Like  the  serous  cavities  it  contains 
normally  a  small  quantity  only  of  fluid,  and  its  size  is  potential 
rather  than  actual. 

The  subarachnoid  space  on  the  other  hand  is,  especially  in 
certain  regions,  such  as  the  dorsal  portions  of  the  vertebral  canal 
and  the  base  of  the  brain,  much  broken  up  by  bridles  of  con- 
nective tissue  passing  from  it  to  the  pia  mater,  as  well  as  by 
a  network  or  sponge-like  arrangement  of  bundles  of  connective 
tissue  lying  immediately  beneath  itself,  and  giving  it  when  viewed 
from  below  a  honeycomb  or  fenestrated  appearance.  The  under 
surface  of  the  membrane  itself  as  well  as  all  the  trabeculae  of  the 
sponge-work  and  the  bridles  are  covered  with  an  epithelium  of 
flat  epithelioid  cells,  which  is  continued  also  over  the  pia  mater 
and  the  ligamentum  denticulatum,  and  lines  the  tubular  sheath- 
like  prolongations  of  the  space  along  the  issuing  nerve  roots. 
The  subarachnoid  space  therefore,  like  the  subdural  space,  may  be 
regarded  as  a  serous  or  large  lymphatic  space,  but  it  is  an  actual 
not  a  mere  potential  space ;  it  always  contains  an  appreciable 
quantity  of  fluid,  which  however  is  not  ordinary  lymph,  but  is 
furnished  in  a  particular  way,  and  deserves  special  study.  To 
understand  the  nature  and  origin  of  this  cerebro-spinal  fluid,  as  it 
is  called,  we  must  turn  to  some  special  arrangements  of  the  pia 
mater. 

§  694.  The  pia  mater  proper,  consisting  of  interwoven  bundles 
of  connective  tissue,  with  some  elastic  fibres  and  a  considerable 
number  of  connective  tissue  corpuscles,  serves  as  we  have  said  as 
the  bearer  of  blood  vessels  to  the  nervous  structures  which  it 
invests.  The  small  arteries  as  they  pass  into  the  nervous  substance 
by  the  way  of  the  septa  are  surrounded  by  peri  vascular  lymphatic 
canals  with  which  spaces  in  the  neuroglial  groundwork  both  of 
the  brain  and  spinal  cord,  especially  spaces  surrounding  the  larger 


CHAP,  ii.]  THE   BRAIN.  1127 

nerve  cells,  are  continuous.  As  is  the  case  with  other  tissues,  so 
with  the  central  nervous  system,  the  several  elements  of  the  tissue 
are  bathed  with  lymph  derived  from  the  blood ;  and  this,  oozing 
through  the  spaces  into  the  perivascular  canals  and  the  other 
lymphatic  vessels  of  the  pia  mater,  makes  its  way  into  the  sub- 
arachnoid  space ;  but  the  fluid  in  the  subarachnoid  space  has  other 
sources  besides. 

The  roof  of  the  fourth  ventricle  is,  as  we  have  said  (§  601) 
reduced  to  a  single  layer  of  non-nervous  columnar  epithelium, 
which  appears  as  a  mere  lining  to  the  pia  mater  overlying  it.  In 
the  hinder  part  of  the  ventricle  this  roof  is  perforated  by  a 
distinct  narrow  oval  orifice,  the  foramen  of  Majendie.  By  this 
orifice,  which  passes  right  through  both  the  pia  mater  and  the 
underlying  layer  of  epithelium,  the  cavity  of  the  fourth  ventricle, 
and  so  the  whole  series  of  cavities  derived  from  the  original 
medullary  canal,  the  lateral  and  third  ventricles,  the  aqueduct, 
and  the  central  canal  of  the  spinal  cord,  are  made  continuous  with 
the  subarachnoid  space.  There  are  also  other  less  conspicuous 
communications  between  the  subarachnoid  space  and  the  fourth 
ventricle.  Hence  the  cerebro-spinal  fluid  is  made  common  to 
all  these  cavities,  and  is  furnished  not  only  by  the  pia  mater 
investing  the  outside  of  the  brain  and  spinal  cord,  but  also,  and 
indeed  probably  to  a  larger  extent,  by  the  epithelium  lining  the 
several  cavities  of  the  cerebro-spinal  axis,  especially  perhaps  by 
those  portions  of  that  epithelium  which  coat  the  processes  of  pia 
mater  projecting  into  those  cavities  at  certain  places. 

We  saw  previously  (§  602)  that  a  large  fold  of  the  pia  mater, 
carrying  in  with  it  the  thin  non-nervous  epithelium  which  alone 
represents  at  the  place  the  original  wall  of  the  medullary  canal, 
is  thrust  inward  at  the  transverse  fissure  of  the  brain,  beneath  the 
fornix,  to  form  the  velum  interpositum,  thus  supplying  a  roof  to 
the  third  ventricle,  and  that  it  thence  projects  into  each  lateral 
ventricle  as  the  choroid  plexus  of  each  side,  reaching  from  the 
foramen  of  Monro  in  front  along  the  edge  of  the  fornix  to  the  tip 
of  the  descending  horn.  The  velum  being  a  fold  of  the  pia  mater 
consists  theoretically  of  two  layers,  and  between  the  upper  dorsal 
layer  and  the  lower  ventral  layer,  lies  a  thin  bed  of  connective 
tissue  carrying  arteries  forwards  from  the  hind  edge  of  the  corpus 
callosum,  and  similarly  carrying  veins  backwards;  these  vessels 
supply  the  choroid  plexus  with  an  abundant  supply  of  blood.  In 
the  choroid  plexus,  the  folded  pia  mater  is  developed  into  a 
number  of  villus-like  processes,  the  primary  processes  bearing 
secondary  ones.  Each  process  consists,  like  a  villus,  of  a  basis 
of  connective  tissue,  in  which  the  blood  vessels  end  in  close  set 
capillary  loops,  covered  with  an  epithelium.  The  epithelium, 
though  continuous  with  the  rest  of  the  epithelium  lining  the 
lateral  ventricle,  and  thus  as  we  have  said  shutting  off  the  lateral 
from  the  third  ventricle  (except  at  the  foramen  of  Monro),  and 


1128  THE   CEREBRO-SPINAL   FLUID.  [BOOK  in. 

though  like  it  derived  from  the  wall  of  the  original  medullary 
canal,  is  different  in  structure.  Over  the  ventricle  generally  the 
epithelium  consists  of  ordinary  short  columnar,  apparently  ciliated, 
cells,  with  more  or  less  transparent  cell-substance ;  the  cells  over 
the  choroid  plexus  are  cubical,  often  irregular  in  form,  and  their 
cell-substance  is  loaded  with  granules,  some  of  which  are  pigmen- 
tary. They  have  very  much  the  appearance  of  '  active '  secreting 
cells;  and  indeed  a  branched  process  of  the  plexus  may  be 
compared  to  an  everted  alveolus  of  a  secreting  gland,  with  the 
epithelium  outside  and  the  blood  vessels  within.  It  cannot  be 
doubted  that  these  cells  play  an  important  part  in  secreting 
into  the  cavity  of  the  ventricle  fluid  which,  passing  thence  by  the 
foramen  of  Monro  into  the  third  and  so  into  the  fourth  ventricle, 
finds  its  way  by  the  foramen  of  Majendie  into  the  subarachnoid 
space. 

As  the  velum  overhangs  the  third  ventricle  it  sends  down 
vertically  two  longitudinal  linear  fringes,  which,  resembling  in 
structure  the  choroid  plexuses  of  the  lateral  ventricle,  are  called 
the  choroid  plexuses  of  the  third  ventricle.  From  the  roof  of  the 
fourth  ventricle  there  hangs  down  on  each  side  a  similar  linear 
fringe,  the  choroid  plexus  of  the  fourth  ventricle,  which  is 
especially  developed  at  its  front  end  beneath  the  overhanging 
cerebellum.  These  subsidiary  choroid  processes  doubtless  assist 
in  furnishing  cerebro-spinal  fluid,  but  their  share  is  small  compared 
with  that  of  the  main  choroid  plexuses  of  the  lateral  ventricle. 

§  695.  The  Cerebro-spinal  Fluid.  The  specimens  of  cerebro- 
spinal  fluid  which  have  been  examined  as  to  their  composition 
are  not  quite  comparable  with  each  other,  since  while  some  (such 
as  those  obtained  from  cases  where  a  fracture  of  the  base  of  the 
skull  has  placed  the  subarachnoid  space  at  the  base  of  the  brain, 
where  it  is  largely  developed,  in  communication  with  the  external 
meatus,  and  the  fluid  escapes  by  the  ear)  may  be  regarded  as 
normal,  others  (such  as  those  obtained  from  cases  of  hydrocephalus 
where  the  ventricles  contain  an  unusual  quantity  of  fluid,  or  from 
cases  of  spinal  malformations)  must  be  considered  as  abnormal. 
In  most  of  the  more  complete  analyses,  the  fluid  examined  has 
belonged  to  the  latter  class ;  and  the  following  statements  apply, 
strictly  speaking,  to  them  alone. 

With  this  caution  we  may  say  that  cerebro-spinal  fluid  is  a 
transparent,  colourless  or  very  slightly  yellowish  fluid,  of  faint 
alkaline  reaction,  free  from  histological  elements.  The  specific 
gravity  is  about  1010  or  less,  the  amount  of  solids  being  on  an 
average  1  p.c.  Of  these  by  far  the  greater  part,  *8  or  '9  p.c.,  is 
supplied  by  salts,  the  total  quantity  of  which  as  well  as  the 
relative  amount  of  the  several  constituents  being  about  the  same 
as  obtain  in  blood  and  lymph.  The  comparative  deficiency  of 
solids  is  due  to  the  scantiness  of  the  proteids,  which  rarely  exceed 
•1  p.c.  These  are  chiefly  globulin  and  a  form  of  albumose,  or  even 


CHAP,  ii.]  THE   BRAIN.  1129 

peptone ;  albumin  is  said  to  be  generally  absent.  The  fluid,  save 
apparently  in  exceptional  cases,  does  not  clot,  and  contains 
neither  fibrogenous  factors,  nor  fibrin  ferment.  It  very  frequently 
contains  a  substance  which  like  dextrose  reduces  Fehling's  solu- 
tion but  which  is  not  a  sugar ;  it  appears  to  be  pyrocatechin  or  a 
closely  allied  body. 

Seeing  that  a  fluid  of  such  a  composition  is  of  a  different 
nature  from  ordinary  lymph,  furnished  entirely  in  the  ordinary 
way,  we  might  be  inclined  to  infer  that  probably  a  very  large  part 
of  the  whole  mass  of  the  fluid  is  furnished  by  the  secreting 
epithelium  of  the  choroid  plexus.  But  it  must  be  borne  in 
mind,  that  the  foregoing  analyses  refer  chiefly  to  fluid  appearing 
under  abnormal  circumstances,  and  it  would  be  hazardous  to  draw 
any  wide  inference  from  them.  We  have  little  or  no  exact 
experimental  evidence  as  to  how  much  fluid  is  actually  secreted 
by  the  choroid  plexuses ;  and  if  the  fluids  which  have  been 
analyzed  do  represent  a  mixture  of  ordinary  lymph  supplied 
through  the  pia  mater  with  the  peculiar  secretion  of  the  choroid 
plexus  and  cerebro-spinal  canal,  some  further  change  beyond  the 
mere  mingling  of  the  two  fluids  is  needed  to  explain  the  remark- 
able absence  of  albumin  which  has  been  so  strongly  insisted  upon 
by  various  authors. 

§  696.  We  may  fairly  suppose  that  during  life  the  fluid  is 
continually  being  supplied,  from  the  one  source  or  the  other ;  but 
we  have  no  very  exact  knowledge  as  to  the  rate  at  which  it  is 
furnished.  In  the  dog,  the  fluid  has  been  observed  to  escape  at 
a  rate  varying  very  largely  under  different  circumstances,  and 
ranging  from  1  c.c.  in  40  minutes  to  as  much  as  1  c.c.,  in 
6  minutes,  the  total  quantity  discharged  in  24  hours  varying 
from  36  c.c.  to  240  c.c.  In  the  cases  of  fracture  of  the  base 
of  the  skull  mentioned  above,  a  very  considerable  flow  has  been 
frequently  observed ;  but  it  may  be  doubted  whether  the  abnor- 
mal circumstances  of  such  cases  have  not  raised  the  secretion 
above  the  normal.  The  rate  of  flow  was  found  in  the  dog  to 
be  much  increased  by  the  injection  of  substances  (normal  saline 
solution)  into  the  blood,  but  to  be  relatively  little  influenced  by 
artificial  heightening  of  arterial  pressure.  This  has  been  put 
forward  as  indicating  that  the  fluid  is  chiefly  furnished  as  a 
secretion  and  not  as  an  ordinary  transudation  of  lymph ;  but  it 
cannot  be  regarded  as  affording  a  valid  argument.  The  pressure 
under  which  the  fluid  exists  is  also  very  variable;  it  is  closely 
dependent  on  the  vascular  arrangements  of  which  we  shall  have 
to  speak  presently.  In  the  dog  the  average  pressure  has  been 
estimated  at  about  10  mm.  of  mercury. 

If  the  fluid  is  thus  continually  formed  it  must  always  find  a 
means  of  escape.  This  is  probably  supplied  by  the  tubular 
prolongations  of  the  subarachnoid  space  along  the  nerve  roots; 
these  are  continuous  with  the  lymphatic  vessels  of  the  nerves, 


1130  THE   CEREBRO-SPINAL   FLUID.  [BOOK  in. 

and  so  with  the  lymphatics  of  the  body  generally;  and  in  the 
skull,  the  passages  of  this  kind  along  the  cranial  nerves,  especially 
along  the  two  optic  nerves  into  the  orbits,  afford  a  ready  means  of 
escape.  It  is  also  urged  that  some  of  the  fluid  escapes  through  the 
Pacchionian  glands  directly  into  the  blood  of  the  venous  sinuses. 
In  a  dead  body  fluid  introduced  into  the  subarachnoid  space 
through  an  opening  over  the  bulb,  disappears  at  even  a  very  low 
pressure  with  great  rapidity.  The  circumstances  then  are,  how- 
ever, not  the  same  as  in  life ;  and  the  few  experiments  which 
have  been  made  seem  to  shew  that,  during  life,  a  somewhat  high 
pressure  is  required  to  secure  the  escape  of  fluid  introduced  in 
addition  to  that  naturally  secreted.  Thus  it  is  stated  that  when 
in  a  dog  normal  saline  solution  is  introduced  into  the  subarach- 
noid cavity  at  the  lower  end  of  the  spinal  cord  very  little  resorptioii 
takes  place  so  long  as  the  pressure  remains  as  low  as  about  10  c.c. 
of  mercury ;  as  the  pressure  is  increased  beyond  this  resorption 
quickly  increases.  But  it  may  be  doubted  whether  the  resorption 
of  added  fluid  is  a  fair  test  of  the  escape  of  fluid  naturally  present ; 
and  the  experiment  is  of  value  rather  as  shewing  simply  that 
there  are  means  of  escape  than  as  affording  a  measure  of  the  rate 
of  escape.  Besides,  the  immediate  effects  of  applying  pressure  at 
the  caudal  end  of  the  spinal  cord  are  not  the  same  as  those  of 
applying  pressure  within  the  skull. 

The  rate  of  possible  escape  is  not  without  importance  as 
regards  the  mechanical  importance  of  the  cerebro-spinal  fluid. 
Thus  it  has  been  urged  that  when  an  extra  quantity  of  blood  is 
driven  into  the  skull,  any  injurious  intercranial  compression  is 
prevented,  not  only  by  the  transference  of  a  corresponding  quantity 
of  cerebro-spinal  fluid  through  the  foramen  of  Majendie  from 
the  cranium  into  the  spinal  canal,  the  walls  of  which  are  less 
rigidly  complete,  but  also  by  the  direct  escape  of  the  fluid  from 
the  cavity  of  the  skull  along  the  cranial  nerves  in  the  manner 
described.  It  has  also  been  urged  that  the  fluid  at  the  base  of 
the  skull,  in  the  large  subarachnoid  spaces  of  which  it  gathers  in 
larger  quantity  than  elsewhere,  acts  as  a  sort  of  protective  water 
cushion  to  the  delicate  cerebral  substance,  and  that,  in  general, 
the  presence  of  the  fluid  is  mechanically  useful  to  the  welfare  of 
the  brain,  removal  of  the  fluid  by  aspiration  being  said  to  lead  to 
haemorrhage  from  the  pia  mater  and  to  various  nervous  disorders. 
But  our  knowledge  as  to  the  part  which  the  fluid  plays  is  at 
present  very  imperfect ;  and  its  very  peculiar  chemical  characters 
suggest  that  it  has  some  chemical  as  well  at  least  as  mechanical 
functions. 


SEC.  13.     THE   VASCULAR   ARRANGEMENTS   OF   THE 
BRAIN   AND   SPINAL   CORD. 


§  697.  The  blood  vessels  reach  the  nervous  structures  by 
means  of  the  pia  mater.  In  the  spinal  cord  arteries  coming  from 
the  vertebral,  intercostal  and  other  arteries,  and  travelling  along 
the  nerve  roots  join  the  pia  mater,  and  then  through  the  fissures 
and  septa  reach  all  parts  of  the  cord ;  but  as  we  have  previously 
remarked  the  capillary  network  is  much  denser,  and  therefore  the 
blood  supply  much  greater  in  the  grey  than  in  the  white  matter. 
The  veins,  also  gathered  up  along  the  septa  and  fissures  into  the 
pia  mater,  those  coming  from  the  grey  matter  forming,  before  they 
reach  the  external  pia  mater,  a  conspicuous  longitudinal  vein  on 
each  side  of  the  posterior  grey  commissure,  pass  from  the  pia 
mater  to  the  large  venous  sinuses  of  the  dura  mater  and  so  to 
adjoining  veins. 

In  the  brain  two  important  features  of  the  distribution  of  the 
arteries  deserve  special  attention.  In  the  first  place,  the  quad- 
ruple supply  by  the  right  and  left  vertebral  and  internal  carotid 
arteries  is  made  one  by  remarkable  anastomoses  forming  the  circle 
of  Willis.  The  right  and  left  vertebral  arteries  entering  the 
vertebral  canal  at  the  level  of  the  6th  cervical  vertebra,  and 
running  forwards  towards  the  brain,  join  beneath  the  ventral 
surface  of  the  bulb  to  form  the  single  median  basilar  artery. 
This,  after  giving  off  branches  to  the  bulb,  cerebellum,  and  pons 
divides  into  the  right  and  left  posterior  cerebral  arteries.  Each 
internal  carotid  entering  the  skull  reaches  the  base  of  the  brain 
in  the  region  of  the  floor  of  the  third  ventricle,  and,  passing 
ventral  to  and  athwart  the  optic  tract,  gives  off  the  large  and 
important  middle  cerebral  artery  along  the  fissure  of  Sylvius,  and 
then,  turning  forwards  and  towards  the  median  line,  passes  dorsal 
to  the  optic  nerve  to  end  in  the  anterior  cerebral  artery.  Just 
however  as  it  gives  off  the  middle  artery,  it  sends  backwards, 
inclining  to  the  middle  line,  a  relatively  large  branch,  the  posterior 
communicating  artery,  which  joins  the  posterior  cerebral  near  the 
origin  of  this  from  the  basilar  artery.  Moreover,  the  two  anterior 
cerebral  arteries  soon  after  they  have  crossed  the  optic  nerves, 
just  as  they  are  about  to  run  straight  forwards  along  the  frontal 


1132  THE  ARTERIES   OF   THE   BRAIN.         [BOOK  in. 

lobes,  are  joined  together  by  a  short  wide  branch,  the  anterior 
communicating  artery.  In  this  way  the  vertebral  arteries  through 
the  basilar  artery  join  with  the  carotid  arteries  to  form  around  the 
optic  chiasma  beneath  the  floor  of  the  third  ventricle  an  arterial 
circle,  the  circle  of  Willis. 

Blood  can  pass  along  this  circle  in  various  ways ;  from  the 
basilar  artery  along  the  right  posterior  communicating  artery  to 
the  right  internal  carotid,  and  so  by  the  right  anterior  cerebral 
artery  and  anterior  communicating  artery  to  the  left  side  of  the 
circle,  and  similarly  from  the  basilar  artery  along  the  left  side  to 
the  right,  or  from  the  right  or  from  the  left  carotid  through  the 
circle,  to  the  right  hand  or  to  the  left  hand  in  each  case.  Since 
the  channel  of  the  circle  is  a  fairly  wide  one,  the  passage  in 
various  directions  is  an  easy  one ;  all  the  vessels  radiating  from 
the  circle,  including  the  basilar  artery  and  its  branches,  can  be 
supplied  by  the  carotids  alone,  or  by  the  vertebrals  alone,  or  even 
by  one  carotid  or  one  vertebral  alone.  In  this  way  an  ample 
supply  of  blood  to  the  brain  is  secured  in  the  face  of  any  hindrance 
to  the  flow  of  blood  along  any  one  of  the  four  channels. 

In  what  may  perhaps  be  considered  the  usual  arrangement, 
the  calibre  of  the  posterior  communicating  arteries  is  rather 
smaller  than  the  other  parts  of  the  circle,  so  that,  other  things 
being  equal,  most  of  the  vertebral  blood  will  pass  by  the  posterior 
cerebral  arteries,  while  the  carotid  blood  passes  to  the  middle  and 
anterior  cerebral  arteries ;  but  many  variations  are  met  with. 
We  may  also  here  perhaps  call  to  mind  the  fact  that  the  left 
carotid  coming  off  from  the  top  of  the  aorta,  offers  a  more  straight 
path  for  the  blood  than  does  the  right  carotid  which  comes  off 
from  the  innominate  artery. 

Another  special  feature  of  the  arterial  supply  to  the  brain  is 
that  the  three  large  cerebral  arteries,  posterior,  middle  and  ante- 
rior, are  distributed  almost  exclusively  to  the  cortex  and  to  the  sub- 
jacent white  matter,  while  the  deeper  parts  of  the  hemisphere,  the 
nucleus  caudatus,  thalamus  and  the  like,  with  the  capsule  and  other 
adjoining  white  matter  are  supplied  by  smaller  arteries  coming 
direct  from  the  circle  of  Willis,  or  from  the  very  beginnings  of 
the  three  cerebral  arteries.  It  is  stated  that  these  two  systems 
make  no  anastomoses  with  each  other;  but  this  appears  to  vary 
much  in  different  individuals.  We  may  add  that  the  anterior 
cerebral  artery  supplies  the  cortex  of  the  dorsal  aspect  of  the 
frontal  lobe  as  well  as  the  front  and  middle  portions  of  the  whole 
mesial  surface  of  the  hemisphere ;  while  the  middle  cerebral,  always 
large,  is  distributed  to  the  side  of  the  brain,  that  is,  the  parietal 
lobe,  with  the  ventral  part  of  the  frontal  lobe  and  the  dorsal  part 
of  the  temporal  lobe ;  the  posterior  cerebral  supplying  the  rest  of 
the  cortex,  that  is  to  say,  the  occipital  lobe  including  the  hind  part 
of  the  mesial  surface  of  hemisphere,  together  with  the  ventral 
part  of  the  temporal  lobe.  The  distribution  of  these  arteries 


CHAP,  ii.]  THE   BRAIN.  1133 

therefore  does  not  correspond  to  functional  divisions,  for  while  the 
middle  cerebral  supplies  a  large  part  of  the  motor  region,  it  does 
not  supply  the  whole  of  it,  and  does  supply  parts  outside  it. 
Though  the  small  arteries  as  they  run  in  the  pia  mater  on  the 
surface  of  the  cortex  anastomose  freely,  there  is  very  little 
anastomosis  between  the  small  arteries  which  leaving  the  pia 
mater  dip  down  into  the  substance  of  the  brain ;  hence  when 
these  latter  arteries  are  blocked,  the  nutrition  of  the  part  of  the 
cortex  supplied  by  them  is  apt  to  be  impaired. 

§  698.  The  venous  arrangements  of  the  brain  have  very  special 
characters. 

Along  the  upper  convex  border  of  the  sickle-shaped  fold  of  dura 
mater,  the  falx  cerebri,  is  developed  a  large  venous  sinus,  the 
superior  longitudinal  sinus.  This,  triangular  in  section,  increasing 
in  calibre  from  before  backwards,  is  a  sinus,  not  a  vein ;  its  walls 
are  formed  of  nothing  but  connective  tissue  lined  with  epithelium, 
muscular  elements  being  entirely  absent.  Though  its  channel  is 
broken  by  bridles  of  connective  tissue  passing  across  it,  it  possesses 
no  valves,  and  indeed  these  are  absent  from  all  the  sinuses  and 
veins  of  the  brain.  Most  of  the  blood  returning  from  the  cortex 
and  subjacent  white  matter  is  carried  into  this  sinus  by  veins,  the 
mouths  of  which  are  for  the  most  part  directed  forwards,  that  is  to 
say,  against  the  direction  of  the  blood  stream.  Along  the  lower 
concave  border  of  the  falx  is  a  similar  sinus,  the  inferior  longitudinal 
sinus,  which  however  is  small  and  into  which  relatively  few  veins 
open. 

From  the  deeper  parts  of  the  brain,  and  especially  from  the 
choroid  plexus,  blood  is  conveyed  by  the  veins  of  Galen  along  the 
velum  interpositum  to  the  transverse  fissure,  where  the  veins  of 
Galen  join  the  inferior  longitudinal  sinus  to  form  the  straight  sinus. 
This,  running  along  the  line  formed  by  the  intersection  of  the 
vertical  falx  with  the  (more  or  less)  horizontal  tentorium,  joins  the 
end  of  the  superior  longitudinal  sinus  to  form  the  reservoir  or 
cellar,  called  the  torcular  Herophili,  from  which  the  lateral  sinus, 
passing  on  each  side  along  the  convex  border  of  the  tentorium 
and  gathering  veins  from  the  cerebellum  and  hind  regions,  as  well 
as  from  the  base  of  the  brain,  delivers  the  blood  into  the  internal 
jugular  vein. 

It  should  be  added  that  veins  from  the  nose  and,  through  the 
ophthalmic  veins,  from  the  face  join  the  veins  and  sinuses  of  the 
brain,  and  that  the  so-called  emissory  veins  pass  through  the 
cranium  from  the  scalp  to  the  superior  longitudinal  and  lateral 
sinuses. 

The  channels  for  the  venous  blood  of  the  brain  are  therefore 
not  veins  but  sinuses,  not  so  much  tubes  for  maintaining  a  uniform 
current  as  longitudinal  reservoirs,  which  while  affording  an  easy 
onward  path  can  also  be  easily  filled  and  easily  emptied,  and  in 
which  the  blood  can  move  to  and  fro  without  the  restrictions  of 


1134  THE   VENOUS   SINUSES.  [BOOK  in. 

valves.  This  arrangement  is  correlated  to  the  peculiar  surroundings 
of  the  brain,  which  is  not  like  other  organs  protected  merely  by 
skin  or  other  extensible  and  elastic  tissue,  but  is  encased  by  a 
fairly  complete  inextensible  envelope,  the  skull.  As  a  conse- 
quence of  this,  when  at  any  time  an  extra  quantity  of  blood  is 
sent  from  the  heart  to  the  brain,  room  must  be  made  for  it  by 
the  increased  exit  of  the  fluids  already  present.  For  any  pressure 
on  the  brain-substance  beyond  a  certain  limit  is  injurious  to  its 
welfare  and  activity,  as  is  seen  in  certain  maladies,  where  blood 
passing  by  rupture  of  the  blood  vessels  out  of  its  normal  channels 
remains  effused  on  the  surface  of  the  brain  or  elsewhere,  and 
thus  taking  up  the  room  of  the  proper  brain-substance  leads,  by 
'  compression '  as  it  is  called,  to  paralysis,  loss  of  consciousness,  or 
death.  Some  room  may,  as  we  have  seen  (§  696),  be  provided  by 
the  escape  of  cerebro-spinal  fluid  from  the  skull.  But,  within  the 
limits  of  the  normal  cerebral  circulation,  the  characteristic  venous 
sinuses  especially  serve  to  regulate  the  internal  pressure ;  they 
form  temporary  reservoirs  from  which  a  comparatively  large 
quantity  of  blood  can  be  rapidly  discharged  from  the  cranium, 
the  flow  from  the  sinuses  being  greatly  assisted  by  the  low  or 
negative  pressure  obtaining  in  the  veins  of  the  neck  at  each 
inspiratory  movement  of  the  chest. 

§  6.99.  The  supply  of  blood  to  the  brain  seems  at  first  sight 
not  to  correspond  to  the  importance  of  this  the  chief  organ  of  the 
body.  In  the  rabbit  it  would  appear  that  hardly  more  than  one 
per  cent,  of  the  total  quantity  of  the  blood  of  the  body  is  present 
at  any  one  time  in  the  brain,  a  quantity  but  little  more  than  half 
that  which  is  found  in  the  kidneys ;  and  while  the  weight  of  blood 
in  the  brain  at  any  one  time  amounts  to  about  five  per  cent,  of  the 
total  weight  of  the  organ,  being  about  the  same  as  in  the  muscles, 
in  the  kidney  it  amounts  to  nearly  twelve  per  cent.,  and  in  the  liver 
to  as  much  as  nearly  thirty  per  cent.  Making  every  allowance  for 
the  relative  small  size  and  functional  importance  of  the  rabbit's  brain, 
the  blood-supply  of  even  the  human  brain  must  still  be  small ;  and 
making  every  allowance  for  rapidity  of  current,  the  interchange 
between  the  blood  and  the  nervous  elements  must  also  be  small. 
In  other  words,  the  metabolism  of  the  brain-substance  is  of  im- 
portance not  so  much  on  account  of  its  quantity  as  of  its  special 
qualities. 

The  circulation  in  the  brain  may  be  studied  by  help  of  various 
methods.  A  manometer  may  be  connected  with  the  peripheral 
end  of  the  divided  internal  carotid  artery,  a  second  manometer 
being  attached  in  the  usual  way  to  the  central  portion.  Since  the 
peripheral  manometer  records  the  blood-pressure  in  the  circle  of 
Willis  transmitted  along  the  peripheral  portion  of  the  carotid 
artery,  variations  of  pressure  in  the  circle  of  Willis  may  thus  be 
studied ;  and  a  comparison  of  the  peripheral  with  the  central 
manometer  will  indicate  what  general  changes  are  taking  place 


CHAP.  IL]  THE   BRAIN.  1135 

in  the  circulation  through  the  brain.  Thus  a  fall  of  pressure  in 
the  peripheral  manometer  unaccompanied  by  any  corresponding 
fall  in  the  central  manometer  would  shew  that  the  "peripheral 
resistance  "  in  the  brain  was  being  lowered,  in  other  words,  that 
the  vessels  were  being  dilated. 

In  another  method,  in  the  dog,  the  outflow  of  venous  blood 
from  the  lateral  sinus  through  the  posterior  facial  vein  has  been 
measured.  The  freedom  with  which  blood  passes  along  the  sinuses 
justifies  the  assumption  that  the  outflow  through  the  open  vein 
gives  an  approximate  measure  of  the  rate  of  flow  under  natural 
conditions  ;  still  the  results  are  only  approximate,  and  besides,  the 
continued  loss  of  blood  introduces  error. 

A  third  method  is  a  plethysmographic  one.  The  skull  is  made 
to  serve  as  the  box  of  the  plethysmograph  or  oncometer  (§  410) ; 
a  small  piece  of  the  roof  having  been  removed  by  the  trephine, 
a  membrane  is  fitted  to  the  hole,  and  the  movements  of  the 
membrane  are  recorded  by  help  of  a  piston  and  lever  or  directly 
by  a  lever.  In  young  subjects,  the  fontanelle,  or  portion  of  the 
cranium  not  yet  ossified,  may  be  utilized  as  a  natural  membrane, 
and  its  movements  recorded  in  a  similar  manner.  When  the 
instrument  is  fitted  to  the  hole  in  a  water-tight  manner,  this 
method  records  variations  in  internal  pressure ;  and  we  may  take 
it  for  granted,  unless  otherwise  indicated,  that  greater  or  less 
pressure  is  due  to  more  or  less  blood  passing  to  the  brain.  But 
the  amount  of  pressure  brought  to  bear  on  the  recording  in- 
strument will  also  depend  on  the  readiness  with  which  the 
cerebro-spinal  fluid  escapes  from  the  cavity  of  the  skull ;  if  there 
be  a  hindrance  to  the  escape,  or  on  the  other  hand  an  increased 
facility  of  escape,  the  same  increase  of  supply  of  blood  will  produce 
in  one  case  a  less,  in  the  other  a  greater  movement  of  the  lever. 
If  the  membrane  be  attached  loosely  to  the  hole  so  as  to  allow  free 
escape  of  the  cerebro-spinal  fluid,  the  lever  practically  resting 
on  the  surface  of  the  cerebral  hemisphere,  the  method  records 
variations  in  the  dorso-ventral  diameter  of  the  hemisphere,  and 
these  may  be  taken  as  measuring  variations  in  the  volume  of  the 
brain  and  so  in  the  blood  supply.  In  neither  form,  however,  does 
the  method  by  itself  give  us  all  the  information  which  we  want. 
An  increase  of  blood  in  the  brain,  and  therefore  an  expansion  of 
the  brain,  and  so  a  movement  of  the  recording  instrument,  may 
result  either  from  a  fuller  arterial  supply  or  from  hindrance  to  the 
venous  outflow;  the  former  condition  is,  at  least  in  most  cases, 
favourable  to,  the  latter  always  and  distinctly  injurious  to,  the 
activity  of  the  nervous  structures;  hence  the  teachings  of  the 
lever  must  be  corrected  by  a  simultaneous  observation  of  the 
general  arterial  pressure  and  of  the  blood-pressure  in  the  veins  of 
the  neck.  Moreover,  the  argument  which  we  used  (§  417)  in 
reference  to  the  kidney  may  be  applied  here  and  probably  with 
equal  force,  namely,  that  the  value  of  the  blood  stream  for  the 

F.  72 


1136  THE   CIRCULATION   IN   THE   BRAIN.     [BOOK  in. 

nutrition  of  the  tissue  is  dependent  not  alone  on  the  amount  of 
blood-pressure,  but  also  and  especially  on  the  rapidity  of  the 
flow ;  indeed  this  second  factor  is  of  particular  importance  in  view 
of  the  need  of  supplying  the  nervous  elements  with  an  adequate 
interchange  of  gases.  Now  of  the  rapidity  of  flow  the  plethysmo- 
graphic  method  can  give  us  indirect  information  only. 

§  700.  By  one  or  other  or  all  of  these  methods,  certain 
important  facts  have  been  made  out.  The  volume  of  the  brain, 
as  determined  by  the  amount  of  blood  present  in  it,  is  continually 
undergoing  changes  brought  about  by  various  causes.  Each  heart- 
beat makes  itself  visible  on  the  cerebral  as  on  the  renal  plethys- 
mographic  tracing,  and  as  we  have  seen  in  speaking  of  respirationr 
the  diminution  of  pressure  in  the  great  veins  of  the  neck  during 
inspiration  leads  to  a  shrinking,  and  the  reverse  change  during 
expiration  to  a  swelling  of  the  brain.  The  plethysmograph  also- 
shews  variations,  larger  and  slower  than  the  respiratory  undu- 
lations, and  brought  about  by  various  causes,  such  as  the  position 
of  the  head  in  relation  to  the  trunk,  movements  of  the  limbs, 
modifications  of  the  respiratory  movements,  and  apparently  phases 
of  activity  of  the  brain  itself,  as  in  waking  and  sleeping ;  undu- 
lations corresponding  to  the  Traube-Hering  variations  (§  387)  of 
blood-pressure  may  not  unfrequently  be  observed. 

All  the  various  methods  shew  that  the  flow  through  the  brain 
is  largely  determined  by  a  vaso-motor  action  of  some  kind  or 
another.  And  this  we  might  indeed  infer  from  ordinary  expe- 
rience. When  the  head  is  suddenly  shifted  from  the  erect  to  a 
hanging  position,  there  must  be  a  tendency  for  the  blood  to 
accumulate  in  the  cranial  cavity,  and  conversely  when  the  head 
is  suddenly  shifted  from  a  hanging  to  an  erect  position,  there 
must  be  a  tendency  for  the  supply  of  blood  within  the  cranium 
to  be  for  a  while  less  than  normal.  Either  change  of  position,, 
and  especially  perhaps  the  latter,  would  lead  to  cerebral  disturb- 
ances, which  in  turn  would  in  ourselves  be  revealed  by  affections 
of  our  consciousness.  That  a  perfectly  healthy,  and  especially 
young  organism  whose  vaso-motor  mechanisms  are  at  once  effective 
and  delicately  responsive,  can  pass  swiftly  from  one  position  of 
the  head  to  the  other  without  inconvenience,  whereas  those  in 
whom  the  vaso-motor  mechanisms  have  by  age  or  otherwise 
become  imperfect  are  giddy  when  they  attempt  such  rapid 
changes,  is  in  itself  adequate  evidence  of  the  importance  of  the 
vaso-motor  arrangements  affecting  the  circulation  through  the 
brain.  The  several  methods  agree  in  shewing  that  increased 
general  arterial  pressure,  such  as  that  for  instance  induced  by 
stimulation  of  a  sensory  nerve,  leads  to  a  greater  flow  of  blood  to 
the  brain;  the  volume  of  the  brain  is  increased  and  the  venous 
outflow  by  the  lateral  sinus  is  quickened.  Conversely,  a  lowering 
of  arterial  pressure  leads  to  a  lessened  flow  of  blood  to  the  brain. 

Seeing  that  the  cerebral  arteries  have  well-developed  muscular 


CHAP,  ii.]  THE   BRAIN.  1137 

coats,  the  basilar  artery  in  fact  being  conspicuous  in  this  respect, 
one  would  be  led  to  suppose  that  the  brain  possessed  special 
vaso-motor  nerves  of  its  own ;  and  recognising  the  importance  of 
blood  supply  to  rapid  functional  activity  one  would  perhaps 
anticipate  that  by  special  vaso-motor  action,  the  supply  of  blood 
to  this  or  that  particular  part  of  the  brain  might  be  regulated 
apart  from  changes  in  the  general  supply.  The  various  obser- 
vations, however,  which  have  hitherto  been  made  have  failed  to 
demonstrate  with  certainty  any  such  special  vaso-motor  nerves  or 
fibres  directly  governing  cerebral  vessels.  It  would  be  hazardous 
to  insist  too  much  on  this  negative  result,  especially  since  the 
observations  have  been  chiefly  directed  to  the  nerves  of  the  neck, 
the  experimental  difficulties  of  investigating  the  presence  of  vaso- 
motor  fibres  in  the  cranial  nerves  being  very  great.  Still  it  may 
be  urged  and  indeed  has  been  urged  that  the  flow  of  blood 
through  the  brain  is  so  delicately  responsive  to  the  working  of 
the  general  vaso-motor  mechanism  just  because  it  has  no  vaso- 
motor  nerves  of  its  own.  In  such  an  organ  as  the  kidney,  an 
increase  of  general  blood-pressure,  as  we  have  more  than  once 
insisted,  may  or  may  not  lead  to  a  greater  flow  through  the  kidney 
according  as  the  vessels  of  the  kidney  itself,  through  the  action  of 
the  renal  vaso-motor  nerves,  are  dilated  or  constricted ;  and,  as  we 
have  seen,  a  constriction  of  the  renal  vessels  may  be  one  of  the 
contributors  to  the  increased  general  pressure.  In  the  brain,  on 
the  other  hand,  an  increase  of  general  arterial  pressure  seems 
always  to  lead  to  increase  of  flow.  Thus  in  the  Traube-Hering 
undulations  just  mentioned,  the  expansions  of  the  brain  are  coinci- 
dent with  the  rises  of  the  general  pressure,  whereas  in  the  normal 
kidney  and  in  other  organs  the  local  Traube-Hering  undulation 
reverses  the  general  one,  the  shrinkings  are  synchronous  with  the 
rises  of  pressure,  the  local  constriction  being  one  of  the  factors  of 
the  general  rise.  It  is  argued,  that  in  the  absence  of  vaso-motor 
nerves  of  their  own,  the  cerebral  vessels  are  wholly,  so  to  speak,  in 
the  hands  of  the  general  vaso-motor  system,  so  that  when  the 
blood- pressure  is  high  owing  to  a  large  vaso-constriction  in  the 
abdominal  viscera,  more  blood  must  necessarily  pass  to  the  brain, 
and  when  again  the  blood-pressure  falls  through  the  opening  of 
the  splanchnic  flood-gates  (§  173)  less  blood  necessarily  flows  along 
the  cerebral  vessels.  And  indeed  one  may  recognize  here  a  sort 
of  self-regulating  action ;  for  diminishing  the  supply  of  blood  to 
the  vaso-motor  centre  in  the  bulb  acts,  as  we  know,  as  a  powerful 
stimulus  in  producing  vaso-constriction,  and  so  leads  to  a  rise  of 
blood-pressure ;  but  this  very  rise  of  blood-pressure  drives  more 
blood  to  the  brain,  including  the  bulb,  and  thus  the  injurious 
effects  to  the  brain  threatened  by  an  anaemic  condition  are 
warded  off  by  the  very  beginning  of  the  anaemia  itself.  All  these 
advantages  are,  however,  quite  compatible  with  the  coexistence  of 
special  vaso-motor  mechanisms. 


1138  THE   CIRCULATION    IN   THE   BRAIN.     [BOOK   HI. 

§  701.  Moreover  the  flow  of  blood  to,  and  consequent  change 
in  the  bulk  of,  the  brain,  and  indeed  the  flow  of  blood  through 
the  brain,  as  measured  by  the  venous  outflow,  may  be  modified 
independently  of  changes  in  the  general  blood-pressure.  For 
instance,  stimulation  of  the  motor  region  of  the  cortex  quickens 
the  venous  outflow,  without  producing  any  marked  change  in  the 
general  blood-pressure ;  this  feature  becomes  very  striking  at  the 
onset  of  epileptiform  convulsions  when  these  make  their  appear- 
ance. It  is  difficult  not  to  connect  such  a  result  of  functional 
activity  with  some  special  vaso-motor  nervous  arrangement 
comparable  to  that  so  obvious  in  the  case  of  a  secreting  gland. 
Again,  it  has  been  observed  that  certain  drugs  have  an  effect  on 
the  volume  of  the  brain,  quite  incommensurate  with  their  effect 
on  the  vaso-motor  system ;  thus  in  particular  the  injection  into 
the  general  blood  stream  of  a  weak  acid  produces  a  large  and 
immediate  expansion  of  the  brain,  while  the  introduction  of  a 
weak  alkali  similarly  gives  rise  to  similar  considerable  shrinking. 
It  is  suggested  that  these  effects  are  produced  by  the  acid 
or  alkali  acting  directly  on  the  muscular  coats  of  the  minute 
arteries  and  so  leading  to  relaxation  or  contraction  respectively. 
In  treating  of  the  chemistry  of  nervous  substance  (§  72)  we  stated 
that  "  the  grey  matter  of  the  central  nervous  system  is  said  to  be 
slightly  acid  during  life  and  to  become  more  acid  after  death." 
Recent  observations  go  to  shew  that  the  grey  matter  of  the  cortex 
is  faintly  alkaline  during  life  and  under  normal  conditions,  but 
becomes  acid  after  death  or  when  its  blood-supply  is  interfered 
with;  and  it  has  been  urged  that  nervous  grey  matter  like 
muscular  substance  developes  acidity  during  activity,  as  well  as 
upon  death,  the  acidity  being  probably  due  in  each  case  to  some 
form  of  lactic  acid.  And  just  as  it  has  been  suggested  that  the 
dilation  of  the  minute  arteries  of  a  skeletal  muscle,  accompanying 
or  following  the  contraction  of  the  muscle,  is  brought  about  by 
the  acid  generated  during  the  contraction  causing  a  relaxation 
of  the  muscular  coats  of  the  minute  arteries,  so  it  has  been 
suggested  that  a  similar  acidity,  the  product  of  nervous  activity, 
similarly  leads  in  nervous  tissue  to  a  dilation  of  the  vessels  of  the 
part.  The  existence  of  special  vaso-motor  mechanisms  would, 
however,  afford  a  more  satisfactory  explanation  of  these  and  other 
phenomena ;  in  spite  of  the  negative  results  so  far  obtained,  the 
matter  is  obviously  one  needing  further  investigation.  Meanwhile 
we  have  abundant  evidence  that,  however  brought  about,  the  flow 
of  blood  through  the  brain,  and  probably  through  particular  parts 
of  the  brain,  is  varied  in  accordance  with  the  needs  of  the  brain 
itself  and  the  events  taking  place  elsewhere  in  the  body. 


CAMBRIDGE:  PRINTED  BY  c.  j.  CLAY,  M.A.  &  SONS,  AT  THE  UNIVERSITY  PRESS. 


OCX 


RETURN    BIOSCIENCE  &  NATURAL  RESOURSES  LIBRARY 

TO  •••*•     2101  VALLEY  LIFE  SCIENCES  BLDG.     642-2531 


LOArsl'f|RjQp  1  ; 

'   Vl^li-       1 

tZ  ••'•'•/;:••  '.'.   l.]\ 

LOAN 

4 

5 

6 

ALL  BOOKS  MAY  BE  RECALLED  AFTER  7  DAYS 

DUE  AS  STAMPED  BELOW 


UNIVERSITY  OF  CALIFORNIA,  BERKELEY 

FORM  NO.  DDO,  50m,  1 1/94  BERKELEY,  CA  94720