Skip to main content

Full text of "Postilla"

See other formats


a prerer si ere ary ee 
Ge i be SA ee 
LOR them, * 


Sees See) 


+ 
Tat Se Be RR ony, ale op NE A 
Se ee Se ea ee 
eS re er eS 
eek OCA fee 44 
SE : : 3 5 ETS NE ere Ow) 
cee Hi , 
Whe 4 4 DEP al wae ES pe RPO 


n- 


Pe Ie Pee ee 
oe ath ene 


ta ae ae ‘ 
Bi Mate Ch 8 Mad A Sah 


Sates? axle ep ianr 
IE eS 


Seite aes NEO ae Oa he 


neat rs. 3 
i ee eS 


otal 


POSTILLA 


PEABODY MUSEUM 
YALE UNIVERSITY 


NUMBER 143. 10 MARCH 1970 


SHARPNESS OF TEETH IN MAN 
AND OTHER PRIMATES 


R. G. EVERY 


POSTILLA 


Published by the Peabody Museum of Natural History, Yale University 


Postilla includes results of original research on systematic, evolution- 
ary, morphological, and ecological biology, including paleontology. 
Syntheses and other theoretical papers based on research are also 
welcomed. Postilla is intended primarily for papers by the staff of 
the Peabody Museum or on research using material in this Museum. 


Editors: Jeanne E. Remington and Nancy A. Ahlstrom 


Postilla is published at frequent but irregular intervals. Manuscripts, 
orders for publications, and all correspondence concerning publications 
should be directed to: 


Publications Office ; 
Peabody Museum of Natural History 
New Haven, Conn., 06520, U.S.A. 


Lists of the publications of the Museum are available from the above 
office. These include Postilla, Bulletin, Discovery, special publications, 
and available back numbers of the discontinued journal, Bulletin of the 
Bingham Oceanographic Collection. All except Discovery are available 
in exchange for relevant publications of other scientific institutions 
anywhere in the world. 


SHARPNESS OF TEETH IN MAN AND 
OTHER PRIMATES 


ka Gu EVERY 


Centre for the Study of Conflict, 25 Clifton Terrace, Sumner, 
Christchurch 8, New Zealand, and Dept. of Zoology, Univer- 
sity of Canterbury, Christchurch 1, N.Z. 


(Received June 1968) 


ABSTRACT 


Analyses of wear characteristics on the teeth of a young adult 
male baboon, a male native human from New Guinea and a 
male Caucasian from New Zealand are made, and their insepa- 
rable relationship to behavior emphasized. These characteristics 
provide evidence of the evolution of precise anatomical and in- 
nate behavioral tooth-grinding mechanisms specific to the pro- 
duction of sharp teeth. Although the teeth on eruption have some 
degree of morphological sharpness, the tooth-grinding behavior 
perfects this sharpness and subsequently maintains it throughout 
the functional life of the teeth. The male baboon, used here as 
broadly representative of man’s non-hominid relatives and an- 
cestors, has a specialized, sickle-like, vertically oriented upper 
canine, sharpened specifically as a slashing weapon. The lower 
anterior premolars are the honing tools which grind against the 
upper canines in a motion opposite to that of the masticatory 
stroke. These premolars are noticeably specialized for this tooth- 
to-tooth grinding action by their enlarged buccal crown-faces, 
thickened enamel gingival extensions, and by paired roots placed 
perpendicular to the “whetstone” faces. 

In contrast, man’s short-canine condition has evolved to pro- 
vide a specialized, horizontally sharp shearing device. The con- 
tinuous rows of even, constantly sharpened teeth, vertically 
oriented and firmly anchored in jaws which provide greater force 
at the biting teeth, give man the capacity for powerful, lethal, 


POSTILLA 143: 30 p. 10 MARCH 1970. 


2 POSTILLA 


“segmentive’ biting. Thus man’s dentition is seen not as “gen- 
eralized”, and certainly not as “regressed” or “weakened”, but 
as highly specialized. The significance of the short-canine con- 
dition (currently regarded as a diagnostic feature of hominids) 
is not that man has become biologically defenseless, but that the 
hominid dental mechanism has harnessed attritional wear to 
provide a more stable and durably functional weapon. 

It is concluded that tooth-sharpening and related phenomena 
are evidence of innate behavior related to a specialized, viable, 
biological weapon in Homo sapiens, and because this weapon — 
the teeth — is the primary one and has been overlooked it 
emphasizes a corollary: intraspecies use of the teeth is strictly 
controlled by genetical determinants, whereas such control of 
the secondary weapon — the hand — is slight. 


TEETH SHARPNESS IN MAN AND PRIMATES 3 


INTRODUCTION 


The prime evolutionary advantage of mammalian and especially 
primate teeth — their sharpness — has previously received little 
discussion in the literature. The special morphology of these 
teeth, and the fact that they are composed of enamel and dentine 
(the hardest and most durable of biological substances) gives 
them the capacity to penetrate and divide exogenous materials. 
Because the initial morphological sharpness of unworn teeth is so 
advantageous, mechanisms to perfect and maintain this sharpness 
through progressive wear are further advantageous and have 
evolved as important characteristics of mammals ( with very few 
specialized exceptions, e.g., the toothless anteater and baleen 
whale). 

The fossil record suggests that in the evolution of the hominid 
dental system leading up to Homo sapiens there was a period 
of rapid transition in such mechanisms. Apparently with a few 
million years, at the most, there was a switch from the pongid 
condition of vertically sharp upper canines to the condition seen 
in recent hominids of horizontally sharp teeth. As continuous 
rows of short, even teeth assumed the role of weapons, the 
weapon-like action of the dentition changed from slash to bite. 
Moreover, the process of precise attrition-sharpening, (thegosis, 
see below) was extended to include all upper and lower incisors 
and canines. This was a considerable addition to the premolar- 
plus-molar, horizontal shearing blades which emerging hominids 
already possessed. Incision, furthermore, was profoundly im- 
proved by the significant addition of a unique feature — antero- 
posterior (retrusive) shearing; it was no longer a simple vertical 
(orthal) action in the canine-incisor region, such as that seen 
in the non-hominid anthropoids today. 

In previous reports that introduced my _tooth-sharpening 
hypothesis (Every, 1960, 1965), figures were not presented. In 
the present paper, figures are included and the major arguments 
of the hypothesis are discussed; special attention is given to con- 
siderations of dynamic spatial relationships among dental struc- 
tures. Furthermore, it is emphasized that these anatomical 
considerations can be understood only in the light of corre- 
sponding ethological considerations, of which they are an integral 
part. Tooth-sharpening and related phenomena give a new 


4 POSTILLA 


dimension to the study of mammals (particularly). Out of the 
immense amount of detail already emerging, this paper con- 
centrates on that related to Homo sapiens, his hominid ancestors 
and anthropoid relatives. 


FUNCTIONAL ANATOMICAL CONSIDERATIONS 
NON-HOMINID ANTHROPOIDEA 


The baboon (Papio) shows an extreme specialization for vertical 
sharpness of the canine and is therefore chosen to illustrate 
functional anatomical adaptations in cercopithecoids (Figs. 1-3). 

Important features of the baboon upper canine are as follows 
(ig. 1): 


1. Facet striated by wear from attrition’ (the discrete wear 
from the forceful grinding of the surface of one tooth against that 
of its opposite, i.e., tooth-to-tooth contact wear). See also Figure 
4 and discussion. 


2. The absence of wear from abrasion (the diffuse wear from 
friction of exogenous material). Compare with Figure 4. 


3. The thin enamel coating on the palatal surface (indistinctly 
shown on the mesial side of the facet, and on the less extensively 
worn gingival third of the crown). 


4. The relatively thick enamel coating on the labial surface 
(indistinctly shown by the strip of enamel which forms the distal 
edge — the cutting edge — of the crown). 


1 The basic material of this paper was first prepared as the legend and dis- 
cussion of five illustrations (here figured 1-5) and submitted for publica- 
tion in 1964 along with the article “The teeth as weapons; their influence 
on behaviour” (Every, 1965). At that time I used “attrition” and “abra- 
sion”, terms I had differentiated and given specific meaning (Every, 1960). 
Hitherto in the literature these terms were used synonymously. Since then, 
however, I have used a new term, thegosis (from the Greek, thego, to 
whet, sharpen). My argument for the use of thegosis as more appropriate 
to an evolutionary adaptive phenomenon, and taking precedence over 
bruxism (a term poisoned by a current definition of mal-adaption, pathol- 
ogy and myth) is developed in another paper, in preparation. Since num- 
bers of scientists are already familiar with my term thegosis I shall, there- 
fore, use it in the remainder of this paper. 


TEETH SHARPNESS IN MAN AND PRIMATES 5 


5. The concavity of the vertical cutting (distal) edge; it tends to 
a sickle-like formation. The distal edge is also concave when 
viewed directly from behind. This is because the palatal surface 
itself is distinctly concave, and this extends the length of the 
crown. The reflected light in this unretouched photograph, there- 
fore, comes from the apical part of the facet only. 


6. The continuity of the concavity of the thegosis-facet. This 
extends vertically from the gingival border of the facet to the 
apex of the crown. There is no rounding-over (convexity) at the 
apex. The tooth is thus ground to a sharp spike. 


7. The continuity of the thegosis-facet in the mesiodistal (hori- 
zontal) aspect. There is no rounding-over at the distal edge of 
the crown, the blade of which is thus ground to a fine edge. 


An examination of the origin of the attrition-facet of the upper 
canine reveals specializations characteristic of non-hominid 
catarrhines; these facets are adapted for producing and maintain- 
ing sharpness of the weapon. The grinder (the cutting tool, the 
hone or whetstone) is the lower anterior premolar, P3 (Fig. 3). Its 
morphological and structural characteristics make it possible to 
identify this tooth as a specialized sharpening tool. Moreover, 
this is its major function. It is not in any way specialized for 
shearing or sectioning, as is widely believed. The buccal surface 
of the crown of P; is an elongated area of thick enamel which is 
continuous from apex to gingival extension and which forms a 
hard grinding surface relatively unyielding to wear. In all other 
teeth of Papio (except the lower canine at its tubercle — see 
below) there is a gradual thinning of enamel towards the gingival 
edge. Thin enamel extends over the whole of the palatal surface 
of the upper canine, a feature which allows it to yield easily to 
grinding against Ps, and the underlying dentine, when exposed, 
yields even more readily. 

Specialization of this sharpening tool is further evidenced by 
its roots: it has two; the distolingual is heavier than the mes- 
iobuccal, and the line in which they are placed is at right angles 
to the crown’s grinding surface. Such precise arrangements are 
clearly to withstand the force of the laterally directed sharpening 
action. 

Lower anterior premolar root formations, concomitants of the 


6 POSTILLA 


ric. 1, Palatal surface of upper right canine tooth: young adult male baboon. 
This view is inverted so that the direction of its cutting (distal) edge faces 
in the same direction (left) as in Figs. 2 and 3. 


TEETH SHARPNESS IN MAN AND PRIMATES 7, 


STRIATIONS IN DENTINE 
CUT (GROUND) BY THE 
BUCCAL ENAMEL OF 
LOWER ANTERIOR 
PREMOLAR 


EDGE OF 
THICK LABIAL 
ENAMEL: 

THE SHARP 
CUTTING EDGE 


ENAMEL 


DENTINE 


EDGE OF 
THIN PALATAL 


ENAMEL 


Fic. 1. (cont.) 


vertically bladed upper canines, reappear with significant fre- 
quency in Homo sapiens (Tomes, 1923), strongly to suggest an 
origin from long canined ancestors. Earlier hominids, the 
Australopithecus, show this even more distinctly, as they do the 


8 POSTILLA 


Fic. 2. Terminal phase of premolar grinding the canine in young adult 
male baboon. 


TEETH SHARPNESS IN MAN AND PRIMATES 9 


ABRASION ROUNDED 
INCISAL EDGE 
(RIGHT CENTRAL INCISOR) 


CANINE 


DENTINE 


ENAMEL 


ANTERIOR 
PREMOLAR 


Fic. 2. (cont.) 


feature of a distogingival tubercle on the lower canine. The report 
of these (overt) features (Robinson, 1956) does not, however, 
relate their concomitance (see below). 

Contrary to current understanding, the motion of the mandible 
in this tooth-to-tooth wearing action is not as it is in mastication. 
The masticatory stroke, in the terminal phase of the masticatory 
cycle, is an approximation of the teeth, in a medial, i.e., buccal to 


lingual (ectal) movement; it terminates in central position. In 
contrast, the sharpening movement starts with the teeth in central 


10 POSTILLA 


Fic. 3. Beginning phase of canine grinding canine in young male adult 
baboon. 


TEETH SHARPNESS IN MAN AND PRIMATES 11 


ANTERIOR PREMOLAR FACET ON 
DISTAL SURFACE 
OF LATERAL 


INCISOR 


ANTERIOR PREMOLAR CANINE 


CANINE 


THICK 
ENAMEL 


THIN ENAMEL 


TUBERCEB: 
THICK ENAMEL 


GINGIVAL EXTENSION OF CROWN: THICK ENAMEL 
Fic. 3. (cont.) 


occlusion. The mandible is extended laterally, i.e., from lingual 
to buccal (ental) movement, and is concurrently depressed. This 
is effected by action of the external pterygoid muscle on one side 
alone, in combination with the depressor muscles (chiefly the 
digastric) of the mandible (Every, 1965). The strokes are rapid, 
and the sound produced by such grinding in many animals has 
frequently been heard but has been recorded merely as tooth- 
chattering (e.g., van Hooff, 1962); it has, moreover, been inter- 


12 POSTILLA 


preted solely as an action deriving from tension and perhaps as a 
signal — nothing more. 

The audibility of tooth-grinding (chattering) and the lack of 
any such sound in masticatory tooth-to-tooth contact is consistent 
with the proposition that man’s teeth seldom, if ever, meet in 
masticatory or incisive action; they meet when swallowing, but 
even then too lightly to produce significant wear (Jankelson, 
Hoffman, and Hendron, 1953; Yurkstas and Emerson, 1954; 
Anderson, 1955). Attrition (thegosis) does not occur during 
chewing and swallowing because masticatory and incisive strokes 
are terminated, presumably by proprioceptive reflex, just short 
of contact between opposing teeth. The division of exogenous 
material is achieved as the teeth approximate; should the teeth 
make contact no further advantage would be achieved, and the 
production of uncontrolled wear would be a serious disadvantage. 

The presence of the tubercle situated distogingivally on the 
baboon lower canine tooth (Fig. 3) provides a further example 
of morphological, structural, and behavioral specialization which 
has evolved as a result of the advantages of the sharpening proc- 
ess. The previously unexplained function of the tubercle can 
now be understood as a specialization to protect the apex of the 
upper canine in the terminal phase of the grinding stroke. Fig. 
2 depicts this position. At the critical phase, where the apex of the 
canine is poised precariously on the gingival extension of the 
premolar crown, the hazard of maintaining the necessary grinding 
pressure without rounding over, and thus blunting the apex, is 
eliminated by the presence of the tubercle. This tubercle contacts 
the upper canine tooth higher (gingivally) on its crown, an 
arrangement which (with further mandibular action) allows the 
apex to disconnect from the grinder without change of direction 
and thus without damage. The tubercle supports the lateral pres- 
sure at the termination of the grinding stroke, and as a result, 
possesses a thicker coat of enamel than the remainder of the lower 
canine crown in this area. 

Further sharpening of baboon canine apices is effected by 
grinding the lower canine apex against the apex of the upper. 
The beginning of this action (shown in Fig. 3) appears to require 
a shift from lateral (extrusive) action of the mandible to re- 
trusive action but with the mandible still held in a lateral position. 
It is possible, however, that no change of action is necessary and 


TEETH SHARPNESS IN MAN AND PRIMATES 13 


that the two can occur independently. Retrusion is limited by the 
postglenoid tubercle of the temporomandibular joint. There is, 
nonetheless, sufficient freedom in the baboon’s temporomandi- 
bular joint (only slight movement is necessary) to allow some 
forceful contact with the distal surface of the upper canine, and 
thus effect a mutual sharpening of each apex’. 

The apex of the lower canine is further sharpened by yet an- 
other action of the mandible which grinds it against the distal 
surface (shown in Fig. 3) of the upper lateral incisor. This action, 
which affects the mesiolingual surface of the lower canine, is 
concurrent with the beginning of the major weapon-sharpening 
action on the opposite (contralateral) side of the mouth. 

In the baboon, as in many non-hominid Anthropoidea, the 
enamel of the upper canine’s anterior surface is grooved to form 
two vertical columns. The corresponding (anterior) and the op- 
posing (distal) surfaces of the lower canine are not grooved; 
moreover, both the ground (distal and lingual) surfaces have 
only a thin coat of enamel. This arrangement favors sharpening of 
the lower canine apex, yet maintains the continuity of buccal and 
mesial enamel on the upper canine. This is vital to its piercing 
and cutting efficiency. Although the apices of both canines are 
sharpened, it is the upper canine, with its acute blade sharpness, 
(entirely absent in the lower canine), which is the dominant 
weapon. The action of the upper canine in the baboon (reflected 
in the strong nuchal musculature; Every, 1965) is in slashing 
in a downward, backward, and inward direction. Even when 
biting, this distally sharpened blade, shaped as a sickle (see No. 
5 above) and oriented posteriorly, remains exposed; it is the 
crown’s mesial surface which is covered by the overlapping lower 
canine. The upper canine serves as an efficient weapon rather than 
as a grasping organ’. 


2 The postglenoid tubercle is, significantly, absent in many mammals, par- 
ticularly the rodents and lagomorphs. Its absence in the pig is part of this 
animal’s specialized temporomandibular joint which permits retrolateral 
mandibular action — an adaptation for sharpening the lower canine tusk. 


3 This is in marked contrast to the typical carnivore condition where canines, 
aided by a large diastema, double in function as weapons and grasping 
tools, and although their rounded distal surfaces may show vertical ridges, 
these are slight and do not impair the vital — for a carnivore — grasping 
advantage of the relatively blunt, hook-shaped walls. 


POSTILLA 


14 


Fic. 4. Upper left molars of a male human native of New Guinea. 


TEETH SHARPNESS IN MAN AND PRIMATES 15 


a 

S 

= ro) 

O = 
O 
> 

ao (a) 

3SYBASNVUL < 

oe Ll 
WV 
WW 

o 3SYSASNVYL 

fa) 
lJ 
o 
z 
aS 
= 
=) 
O 

(U) ied 

z < 

= —] 

e e) 

ay = 
| 

Q 

> z 

N% 2 

8) 

a Ww 

i") 


WwW 
z 
= 
Zz 
uJ 
a 


Fic. 4. (cont.) 
HOMINIDS 


Figure 4 shows some of the features of wear and sharpening 
processes in the molars of Homo sapiens. The third molar, with 
six to eight years’ less use than the second molar, shows a clear 
picture of wear principally from the tooth-to-tooth contact wear 


16 POSTILLA 


of thegosis. This wear is the result of two discrete mandibular 
movements which have produced two equally discrete sets of 
facets (some of these are high-lighted in the photographs); the 
facets form ridges and grooves which meet at distinct boundaries, 
The transverse boundaries (vertical in the photograph) are 
formed by a left lateral (extrusive) thegosis action. In this action 
the mandible rotates about an axis at the left postglenoid tubercle 
(see discussion of Fig. 5, below), i.e., about a point posterior to 
the mandibular condyle on the same (ipsilateral) side as the 
teeth in the figure. This movement is in exact opposition to that 
of the masticatory stroke in its terminal phase, and therefore must 
occur apart from mastication. The oblique boundaries are formed 
by a lateral mandibular movement, i.e., the mandible rotates 
about an axis at the postglenoid tubercle on the opposite (con- 
tralateral) side. As this movement is beyond the terminal phase 
of a masticatory stroke, the oblique facets are also fashioned 
apart from mastication. 

Wear from abrasion (in this instance, the friction of food) is 
predominant in the second molar because it has had from six 
to eight years’ more use than the third molar; there is, however, 
a small thegosis-facet on the remnant of the distal surface of the 
distopalatal cusp (hypocone). Mesial to this distal thegosis-facet, 
with its discrete transverse ridge, are irregular, but still trans- 
versely oriented, lines. These are the result of friction scouring 
by exogenous material. In contrast to the thegosis-facet, the more 
heavily abraded remainder of the occlusal surface of this second 
molar has no precise occlusal conformity with an antagonist. 

Three other key features are to be seen on this second molar: 


a) The prominent sharp edge to the buccal cusps, from which 
the chewing table slopes down to the less prominent palatal cusps 
(the incline is more apparent in a mesial or distal view). This 
buccal edge is (relative to the movement of the lower molars) 
the leading (cutting) edge of the upper molar; it forms a crest 
on the tooth’s vertical, relatively flat, buccal surface. In the lower 
molars the corresponding leading edge and vertical, relatively 
flat, surface is lingual. 

b) The worn concavity of the palatal aspect of the buccal cusps. 
This feature, which helps to maintain the sharpness of the lead- 
ing edge, is formed as a result of specializations in the temporo- 


TEETH SHARPNESS IN MAN AND PRIMATES 17 


mandibular joint, in the mandibular symphysis, and in the 
proportions of the basal skull, maxillae, and mandible. 

c) The remnant of the oblique ridge connecting the distobuccal 
cusp with the mesiopalatal cusp. Midway along the palatal section 
of this ridge is a small area of exposed dentine. With progressive 
wear from the friction of masticatory action other areas of dentine 
are exposed and gradually increase in size, and because dentine 
is a softer substance these areas become hollowed out. This 
phenomenon is characteristic of most mammals and provides a 
most important adaptive feature in allowing additional enamel 
cutting edges to appear (i.e., around the boundaries of the 
abraded dentine), thus maintaining — even enhancing — the 
sharpness of the tooth throughout its life. 


Figure 5 shows some of the features of wear and sharpening 
processes on the incisors and canines of Homo sapiens. The 
principle reason for inclusion of this figure is to demonstrate the 
incorrectness of the hypothesis that states that excessive tooth 
wear in civilized man is the result of a “heavy bite”. This hypo- 
thesis also often includes the proposition that heavy wear may 
have atavistic components, occurring more frequently in cul- 
turally primitive races (the traditional example given is the 
Australian aborigine ). 

Heavy wear is most often accounted for (e.g., Zuckerman, 
1958) by assuming that use (age) wears hominid teeth down 
from their tips, and that they become flat and blunt as a result. 
Fig. 5, however, clearly shows a picture of wear which is domi- 
nated by thegosis, with the remaining loss of tooth substance 
resulting from decay, abrasion, and erosion. There is also evi- 
dence that thegosis occurs on distinctly separate occasions from 
incision and mastication, i.e., in the absence of exogenous ma- 
terial. Furthermore, it is possible to determine that the mandibular 
stroke producing thegosis is both oblique to the incisive stroke, 
and extrusive. Its action is in contrast to that of the terminal 
phase of incision, which is directly retrusive. This is shown by: 


1) The thegosis ridges on the right (left side of the picture) 
canine, and (indistinctly) on the right central incisor. These 
thegosis ridges form arcs which are concentric with the trans- 
verse ridges on molars situated on the same side (ipsilateral), 


POSTILLA 


Fic. 5. Three remaining incisors, two canines, and one premolar from the 
upper jaw of a Caucasian male New Zealander. 


19 


TEETH SHARPNESS IN MAN AND PRIMATES 


JISSNW AIODAN3SLd IWNYS1LX3 


31794¥30dNL GION3191S0d 


vVSSO4d GION3A19 


DYV 3ANdIIGO 


~ 


SNILN3G 
a 

TAWYN3I aes 
NOILVYOLSSY AON1IV d109 
(ANIVIGV1) 39037 037704 

(J79NV 3SNL8O) 3903 AVLVIVd ONITIVYL SHL 


(J19NV 3LNDV) 
3903 ONILLND BHL :3903 AvIGV1 ONIGV3T J3HL 


(AIGIGNVW SHL 40 
LNAWSAOW AYOLVISNVYL 
SSYSASNVYL LHOMS 
Vv Sl 3Y3HL +-379Y4REnNL 
SHL OL YOlW3LSOd 
SS XN SV MES Vw ell ERIE) 


NOILvLOY 4O SIXV 


| 
DYV ASYSASNVYL 


AVIIAG 


SNILN30 NI 
TAWVYNA Ni 
‘NOISOYUZ BF NOISVYEEV 


EID) | 
g3113A3a 
“NOILIYLLV 


Fic. 5. (cont.) 


and with the oblique ridges on the opposite (contralateral) side 


of the dental arch. 


2) The gold-alloy restoration of the mesial incisal angle of the 


left lateral incisor which has the appearance of being repeatedly 


20 POSTILLA 


beaten. The wear on all the anterior teeth shows that their oc- 
clusal relations to the lower anterior teeth are now relatively 
edge-to-edge; there is evidence of an overjet but no overbite. If 
the current notion were true — that normal wear on anterior 
teeth is a result of incision — this action would be expected to 
produce a palatal rolling over of the malleable alloy, since the 
terminal phase of incision is a retrusive mandibular stroke (par- 
ticularly in this edge-to-edge specimen where there is no over- 
bite). But the alloy is rolled to a labially projecting ledge. It has 
been hammered over to produce a ledge, as in a rivet. This can 
only have occurred under an action of forceful occlusion — an 
action distinct from and incompatible with incision or mastica- 
tion (Every, 1965). 

3) The flat incisal facet on the right central incisor. Especially 
when there is a normal incisor overbite and overjet relationship, 
this characteristic flat formation can be caused only by a mandi- 
bular movement which extensively crosses over the two rows 
(upper and lower) of anterior teeth. That is, the row of lower 
anterior teeth must cross over, and become diagonally related to, 
the row of upper anterior teeth. This is actually achieved by 
extensions of the same movement which grinds facets on the 
molars. (See evidence outlined in discussion of Fig. 4.) 

4) The angle that a typical upper incisal facet makes with the 
vertical labial surface is about 76°. (Although this is not apparent 
in the figure, the incisal facet is inclined palatally). The fallacy 
that these teeth wear blunt is exposed by noting that the flat 
facet is the bevel to the sharp, leading, labial edge. In the lower 
teeth, the sharp, leading, cutting edge is the lingual boundary 
of the incisal facet. In contrast to man, the baboon’s incisors (see 
Figs. 2 and 3) are more likely to wear blunt; but this is an abra- 
sive bluntness: the baboon cannot grind bevelled facets on its 
incisal edge — any possible crossing over of occluding upper and 
lower rows of anterior teeth is prevented by the baboon’s long 
canines and jaw proportions. Furthermore, relative to the pongid 
condition and, by inference, that of the hominid’s long-canine- 
toothed ancestors, the lingual enamel which forms the sharp 
cutting edge of lower incisors and canines is thickened. Although 
the corresponding sharp edges of upper incisors and canines are 
labial, the palatal enamel of these teeth is also thickened. This, 
however, is to the advantage of the stage of wear when dentine 


TEETH SHARPNESS IN MAN AND PRIMATES 21 


becomes exposed and hollowed out by abrasive wear (see dis- 
cussion of Fig. 4). When this occurs the thickened palatal enamel 
provides an additional cutting edge, and is also subject to 
thegosis. This feature, of course, correspondingly occurs, but in 
the reverse relationship, in the lower teeth; the new cutting edge, 
which appears when dentine is exposed, forms a crest on the 
labial enamelt. 


DIscussIon 


When man fabricated his first stone chisels, he found an optimum 
angle of bevel which was advantageous in cutting and durability. 
He had probably learned much from his use of other animals’ 
teeth as chisels. In his use of either stone or teeth he would not 
have directly applied the flat bevel in the cutting stroke. Yet this 
inefficient action is imagined by some to occur in the functioning 
of man’s own teeth; the flat facet on incisors of Homo sapiens is 
called the “incisal edge”, whereas the real incisal edge is the 
boundary of the facet (the edge of the bevel), not its flat surface. 

Hundreds of millions of years before man fabricated a function- 
ing blade — a blade which, when blunted by use, was resharpened 
— natural selection had achieved numerous specializations for 
the maintenance and function of sharp teeth. It is a common- 
place that, failure to discern a specific function for an anatomical 
feature does not imply that it is truly functionless. The “function- 
less” lateral incisors of the lagomorphs, for example, actually 
serve as specialized sharpeners; this is their dominant, if not 
exclusive, function (Every, 1967). By a stroke of the mandible at 
right angles to the incisive stroke an extremely fine “razor-edge” 
to the lower central incisor is honed’. The precision of this action 
in lagomorphs becomes even more apparent when it is seen that 


4 This system of consecutive blades oriented relatively in a horizontal plane 
is seen more specifically in the selenodont molars of herbivores. Selenodont 
teeth only come into full function when dentine is exposed and hollowed 
out and the precise shearing blades (not rough, grinding surfaces) are 
brought into accurate alignment by thegosis — a genetically programmed 
behavior as in most other mammals. A characteristic selenodont molar then 
presents pairs of four consecutive, sickle-like blades, and each of the eight 
blades is precisely oriented to face the direct line of the masticatory stroke. 


5 The leading edge of the lower central incisor of lagomorphs and rodents 
in particular, and of most mammals in general, is labial. 


22 POSTILLA 


the striations of the remainder of the extensive bevel are cut by the 
twin gouges which crest the incisal edge of the labial surface of the 
upper central incisor. This action involves a forceful protrusive 
mandibular stroke. Thus the two sets of striations on each bevel 
of the lower incisors are at right angles to each other and cannot 
have been caused either by grasping or by cutting of exogenous 
material. In marked contrast to this wear on the lower incisors is 
the characteristic wear from abrasion on the upper incisors, which 
likewise contributes to sharpening. (A detailed analysis of this 
will be presentéd in a later paper. ) 

The significance of all this to man is that it clarifies not only 
human dental morphology but also his innate tooth-sharpening 
behavior. It is important to emphasize that, for an animal to kill, 
two basic components must exist: a) the anatomical weapon, and 
b) the physiological mechanisms to activate it. These two com- 
ponents, though separate conceptually, are in functional terms 
inseparable. Aggressive behavior is unlikely to evolve, ie., be- 
come genetically programmed, when there is no _ biological 
weapon. Learned improvements in aggressive behavior, more- 
over, are still less likely to occur in such a case. 

In this light the hypothesis that the earliest hominid was 
biologically defenseless, and that improvements in the brain and 
hand produced aggressive behavior and cultural weapons “in 
compensation’, is unconvincing. 

The important part played by teeth in the evolution of most 
mammals is generally recognized, and particularly in the evolu- 
tion of the hominids the teeth are considered to have played a 
vital part. But, paradoxically, this part is considered to be a 
negative one — the weapon is thought to have disappeared. In the 
light of the phenomenon of thegosis, however, these erroneous 
notions can now be discounted. The clarification of man’s evolu- 
tion concerns not only the evolution of his biologically inheritable 
features but also the evolution of his culture, which he does not 
inherit but acquires. 

These subsequently learned cultural improvements in killing, 
however, today so occupy our attention that we tend to be ob- 
livious of any unlearned components which may be programming 
our behavior. Any suggestion that innate aggression is a com- 
ponent of, human behavior we tend to regard as objectionable, as 
“animal” and “inhuman”. And, at best, when its existence is recog- 


TEETH SHARPNESS IN MAN AND PRIMATES 23 


nized, we hopefully believe that we may effect its control by 
avoidance, redirection, and sublimation. 

It seems that the phenomenal success of our (learned) cultural 
achievements has caused us to fail to perceive the importance of 
precultural improvements in learning as facilitating genetical 
programming of behaviors. The increased capacity to learn is, of 
course, a biological improvement. The cultural improvement 
(particularly the overwhelming advantage of the accumulation 
of experience through the capacity to speak) is merely an ex- 
tension of the same advantage. It is as if learning were the leaven 
to the dough: leaven does not constitute the food; it merely im- 
proves the food and enhances its ultimate utilization. 

Interpretations of these hitherto unsuspected phenomena re- 
quire reappraisals of both the palaeontological and the recent 
record of reptilian and mammalian life. In the case of man, the 
reappraisal includes not only aspects of his morphological charac- 
teristics, of his behavior and social organization (of those aspects 
which are genetically determined), but also of his culture — the 
feature which distinguishes him from other animals. 

Furthermore, these interpretations are antithetical to many 
current ideas of the selective forces which produced man. They 
suggest, not merely a modification, but a reversal of ideas, par- 
ticularly related to man’s aggression. Certain notions, such as 
those aptly stated by Washburn (1960), become untenable: 
“The skull of the man-ape has transferred to its hands the func- 
tions of seizing and pulling, and this has been attended by a 
reduction of its incisors. Small canines and incisors are biological 
symbols of a changed way of life; their primitive functions are 
replaced by hand and tool.” 

Such misinterpretations of the relative grasping capacities of 
the dentition of higher primates can be disproved in the light of 
phenomena already well documented. For example: the assump- 
tion that there had been a transfer, in the “man-ape’, of functions 
of seizing and pulling (to hands from teeth), suggests that no 
such transfer had occurred in the ape itself; yet I have seen no 
report of anyone having observed an ape transporting its young 
by its teeth, let alone supporting its own weight by grasping with 
its teeth; nor any report of any ape seizing and grasping an ad- 
versary, or a struggling prey, by its teeth. In contrast, the seizing 
and pulling capacity of man is demonstrated by his capacity to 


24 POSTILLA 


seize and hold an adversary by his teeth, and by the circus per- 
former’s act where the force from the combined weights of two 
individuals, plus the centrifugal force from their swinging as a 
pendulum, is supported by the strength of one dentition. 

Such a capacity is made possible in man by the improved 
leverage of his jaws as a result of the shortening of the snout. It 
is a feature which has allowed the reduction in the size of the 
“masticatory” muscles without loss of force available at the 
teeth (Every, 1965). 

The grasping capacity of the sub-human primate dentition is 
impaired, furthermore, by the acute sharpness of the distal edges 
of the upper canine teeth. These canines do not double in func- 
tion as grasping tools as do the carnivore’s; they are specialized 
as slashing weapons, and are more formidable than the carni- 
vore’s, which are poorly adapted for slashing. The grasping 
function of a carnivore’s canine teeth, however, is enhanced by 
the bluntness of the walls of their crowns and by diastemata 
posterior to, and thus exposing, their bluntness. Although the 
slashing advantage is to the detriment of grasping, this is of little 
consequence to a primate; the arboreal ancestors of all primates 
show no evidence of their teeth having had dominant grasping 
functions. 

Washburn’s statement quoted above that the transference of 
functions had been “attended by a reduction of [the man-ape’s] 
incisors” suggests that there is a correlation between these factors, 
i.e., large incisors are advantageous for seizing and pulling. Yet 
the dentitions of characteristic carnivores, specialized for seizing 
and pulling, have minute incisors, relatively a fraction of the 
size of those of any anthropoid, including man. 

Until the tooth-sharpening hypothesis first appeared (Every, 
1960), there was, apparently, no suggestion that the hominid’s 
short canine teeth gave direct evolutionary advantage per se 
to their possessor. On the contrary, evolutionary theorists widely 
and confidently held that the canines had “regressed” and had 
become “weak and inefficient”. Moreover, they believed that, in 
use, the canines “wore down from their tips” and soon become 
“flat and blunt” — as did the hominid incisors, premolars, and 
molars (e.g., Zuckerman, 1958; Leakey, 1960; Le Gros Clark, 
1962). This was taken as evidence that biological progress in 
early hominids had occurred in other features, particularly in the 


TEETH SHARPNESS IN MAN AND PRIMATES 25 


brain and in the use of the hand, and that these morphological 
and cortical adaptations were “necessary” to offset the disadvan- 
tages of short, small, and weak teeth. This theory suggests that 
Ramapithecus, and certainly the earliest Australopithecus, must 
have had sufficient intelligence to use tools, if not to make them. 
It also suggests that this capacity must have been developed 
sufficiently to compensate for the absence of dental weapons dur- 
ing millions of years of what must on this theory have been ex- 
tremely precarious existence. It is clear, however, that during 
the Pleistocene there was no lack of sizable predators. Early 
hominids were not fast runners; they had, as a result of biped- 
alism, a reduced climbing capacity, a low procreative rate, a 
reduced sense of smell and hearing, probably a lengthening 
period and increasing intensity of infant dependence, a small 
brain, no capacity to transmit accumulated experience by speech, 
and a comparatively limited capacity to transmit any experience 
by signals. In addition, evolutionary theorists widely held that the 
teeth were inefficient, not only as weapons, but also as tools of 
mastication, incision, and grasping. Despite all these disadvan- 
tages and hazards, early hominids were not overwhelmed; they 
managed to survive the long and critical epoch unprotected by 
the intelligence which is concomitant with a capacity for true 
speech, and supposedly unprotected by an effective biological 
weapon. This supposed achievement was made the more re- 
markable by the absence of one other significant and fundamental 
advantage which a biological weapon gave and still gives today. 
This advantage, moreover, is one which no artificial weapon pre- 
sents or can ever present. It is that a biological weapon is built 
in; it cannot be dropped, mislaid, or lost, nor can its possessor 
be dispossessed, or taken by surprise “unarmed”; in an emergency 
it is always immediately available. 


CONCLUSIONS 


The evidence for the hypothesis of man’s biological killing capac- 
ity can be studied in three aspects: 


1. The anatomical weapon, i.e., the biological instrument of 


killing. 


26 POSTILLA 


2. The permanent (inflexible) genetical determinants of killing 
behavior. 

3. The transient (flexible) learned adjustments to the genetically 
programmed behavior. 


The simple but fundamental hypotheses of tooth-sharpness and 
tooth-sharpening processes (thegosis) throw further light on the 
evolutionary events leading to man. The evidence suggests that 
in hominid evolution there was no period of defenselessness; the 
increased number of attrition-sharpened teeth, introducing the 
new and specialized feature of anterior shearing blades with a 
capacity for “segmentive biting”® (Every, 1965), was defensively, 
predatorily, and aggressively advantageous. It was especially 
advantageous when coupled with an increasing capacity to know 
and to signal when and where not to use the primary biological 
weapon. 

The origin of the short canine by paedomorphic novelty is 
generally accepted (e.g., Koestler, 1966). Also accepted is the 
significance to evolutionary processes of paedomorphic novelty, 
which is not so much the initial event itself but that selection, here 
(operating on a more plastic, less committed stage) allows a 
sudden advance in a new direction. It is this sudden (now 
gerontomorphic) advance which, in the hominid short-canine 
condition, evolutionists have overlooked. 

Once this process advanced to the stage where the relatively 
sudden appearance of a chin altered the shape of the oral cavity 
and the face, these prior advantages permitted the evolution of 
the further, and overwhelming, advantage of a capacity to speak 
(Every, 1965). 

But along with this overwhelming advance in the capacity to 
learn came an inevitable disruption of the biologically balanced 
(unlearned) controls: the physiological reactions evoking and 
attenuating agonistic behavior. This disruption clearly resulted 
from two principal causes: 


1. The introduction of the exogenous (artificial) weapon im- 
mediately gave lethality to the secondary, and fundamentally 
non-lethal, weapon — the hand. Being non-lethal (except by 


6 I use this term (Every, 1965) to describe the separation and removal (in 
one action) of a large chunk of material. 


TEETH SHARPNESS IN MAN AND PRIMATES 27 


accident) the hand is subject to relatively scant intraspecies 
(ritualized ) control of its use, whereas the lethal primary weapon 
— the teeth — has its intraspecies use strictly controlled. That this 
control of agonistic behavior should be built in, i.e., automatic, 
stereotyped, species-specific (universal) and unlearned, is 
clearly advantageous. For, in agonistic behavior, especially when 
the weapon is sexually dimorphic (Every, 1965) in the adult, 
there is no margin of time allowance for learning: uncontrolled, 
random, trial-and-error actions of a built-in lethal instrument 
would rapidly lead to chaos. And if the species were to survive 
there would need to be strong selective pressure against learning 
the controls. This feature accounts for the restricted use of the 
primary weapon today, i.e., it is restricted in frequency of oc- 
currence, force of bite, and selection of site; seldom are teeth 
used, seldom is a bite segmentive, and it is almost unknown for it 
to be sited at the fatally vulnerable neck. 

2. The introduction of the exogenous weapon immediately ex- 
tended the distance from the attacker in which the adversary 
could be injured. With sophistication of the weapon and pro- 
gression of the distance this concomitantly and progressively 
dilutes the efficacy of the biological perceptions (seeing, hearing, 
touching, smelling, tasting) through which the controls are in- 
strumented. Today, a kill can be made in circumstances devoid 
of direct biological perception. There is, as a result, almost com- 
plete disruption of the biologically balanced controls. The evoker, 
inevitably and tragically, is favored. 


Genetical determinants of an animal’s behavior can be con- 
sidered permanent in the sense that they evolve in relation to 
permanent, or even relatively permanent, environmental features 
such as seasons, tides, bisexual reproduction, prey-food, and so 
on. Even less consistent but reasonably cyclic features such as 
droughts and famine can, in this sense, qualify as “permanent”. 
When this permanency, i.e., inflexibility, is applied in the con- 
sideration of innate controls of killing behavior, whether it be 
predation, defense from predation, or intraspecies decimation in 
caged (e.g., overpopulation) circumstances, it is clear that 
especially for the higher animal, the capacity to learn is crucial. 
Because of the ever present non-permanent (i.e., transient) en- 
vironmental circumstance, learning allows adjustment to the 


28 POSTILLA 


timing and the movement of the innate sequence, and thus facili- 
tates its consummation. In this view the capacity to learn is an 
inherent concomitant of the unlearned determinant of behavior 
— not a replacement. 

Direct and comparative morphological studies of man’s denti- 
tion, together with dental’, psychological®, psychosomatic’, 
vertebrate paleontological’®, and paleoanthropological™ studies 
of the conditions which evoke his sharpening behavior, suggest 
that man sharpens his teeth as weapons; he does not directly 
sharpen his teeth as tools of mastication or grasping. His teeth 
are adequately maintained as sharp tools in the normal frequency 
of their preparation as weapons. 

The tooth-sharpening phenomenon uniquely presents a discrete 
entity of innate behavior in man which appears to be unclouded 
by argument that it could be learned. It, moreover, elucidates un- 
learned determinants of man’s agonistic behavior, and strongly 
suggests that a partial measure of the (pre-cultural) biologically 


7 Experimental studies in loss (other than by caries and accident) of tooth 
substances; force and direction of mandibular action; i.e., using and de- 
vising techniques of conservative restoration, periodontia, therapeutic 
splinting and orthodontic appliance, full and partial prosthesis (allowing 
unique control of nocturnal action). (Every, 1939, 1949, 1960, 1965; 
Craddock and Johnston, 1961; Reed, 1968). 


8 Communication by facial expression, particularly (innate) signals and their 
auditory concomitants. Communication by olfaction. Relationship of 
(innate) unlearned and (cultural) learned determinants of behavior. 
Behavioral and morphological changes permitting the evolution of the 
capacity to speak. Pain thresholds in fight and flight. Use of tooth-sharpen- 
ing as a marker of stress. (Every, 1960, 1965; Hughes, 1969). 


9 Oral symptoms of repression. Presentation of the Syndrome of extreme 
mandibular movements. Experimental therapy. (Every, 1946, 1960; Crad- 
dock and Johnston, 1961). 


10 The reptilian-mammalian transition. Mesozoic mammalian dentition, par- 
ticularly in respect to the origin of the talonid and protocone, the contact 
point, the transition of cusp-interdigitation to cusp-apposition and 
opposing-convexities of shearing blades, and the significance to sharpness 
of differentially hard tooth-substances. Post-Mesozoic radiation. (Every, 
“Reinterpreting the dentitions of Amphitherium and Peramus,” paper read 
at the Symposium of Vertebrate Palaeontology and Comparative Anatomy, 
London, 1967). 


11 The transition from vertically sharp slashing blades to horizontally sharp 
shearing blades. Evolutionary advantages of the hominid short-canine 
condition. Relationship of changes in the dentition to brain enlargement. 
(Every, 1965, and paper cited in Footnote 10). 


TEETH SHARPNESS IN MAN AND PRIMATES 29 


balanced controls is restorable. But this only when man’s 
(flexible) culture is made to harmonize with his (inflexible) 
biology. 


ACKNOWLEDGMENTS 


Dr. Elwyn L. Simons (Peabody Museum of National History, 
Yale University) has given much assistance in the preparation 
of this manuscript, for which I am deeply grateful. Others also 
deserve my sincere gratitude; they gave facilities, access to col- 
lections and clinical material, discussion, service, encouragement 
and support. Too numerous to mention individually, they are 
spread out among universities, museums, hospitals, miscellaneous 
institutions, private practices, and private individuals, in New 
Zealand, Australia, Germany, England and the U.S.A. To Pro- 
fessor Dr. Walter G. Kiihne, Geologisch Palaontologisches In- 
stitut, Freie Universitat, Berlin; Dr. Fraser McDonald, Medical 
Superintendent and Director Research Unit, Kingseat Psychiatric 
Hospital, Papakura, New Zealand; Dr. Ronald Singer, Dept. of 
Anatomy, University of Chicago, and Dr. Albert A. Dahlberg, 
Dept. of Anthropology and Zoller Memorial Dental Clinic, Uni- 
versity of Chicago, I give special thanks for the additional assis- 
tance of short-term posts in their departments. The whole basis 
of this work was formulated in a psychiatrically oriented private 
dental practice in Christchurch, N. Z. In the final stages, crucial 
financial support, most of it anonymous, was given by private 
well-wishers. For this I am extremely grateful. Part of the prepar- 
ation for publication was later supported by grants from the 
Canterbury Medical Research Foundation, N.Z.; The Golden 
Kiwi Lottery Committee for the Promotion of Medical Research, 
N.Z.; and The Explorers Club, New York, U.S.A. 


LITERATURE CITED 


Anderson, D. J. 1955. Physiology of mastication. Dent. Pract. 5: 389-394. 
Craddock, F. W., and J. W. Johnston. 1961. A new explanation of facial 
pain. N.Z. Dent. J. 57: 153-161. 
Every, R. G. 1939. Contact points. N.Z. Dent. J. 35: 287-291. 
1946. The problem of the thumb-sucker. N.Z. Dent. J. 42: 
178-183. 


30 POSTILLA 


1949. The elimination of destructive forces in replacing teeth 
with partial dentures. N.Z. Dent. J. 45: 207-214. 
1960. The significance of extreme mandibular movements. 
Lancet 2: 37-89. 
__ 1965. The teeth as weapons; their influence on behaviour. 
Lancet I: 685-688. 

Hughes, R. N. 1969. Social facilitation of locomotion and exploration in 
rats. Br. J. Psychol. 60: 385-388. 

Jankelson, B., G. M. Hoffman, and G. A. Hendron. 1958. Physiology of 
the stomatognathic system. J. Am. Dent. Assn. 46: 375-386. 

Koestler, Arthur. 1966. Biological and mental] evolution — an exercise in 
analogy, p. 95-107. In Knowledge among men. Simon and Schuster, 
Washington, D.C. 

Leakey, L. S. B. 1960. Finding the world’s earliest man. Nat. Geog. 118: 
240-435. 

Le Gros Clark, W. E. 1962. The antecedents of man, 2nd ed., University 
Press, Edinburgh. 388 p. 

Reed, J. W. 1968. A new design for a partial lower denture. N.Z. Dent. J. 
64: 23-26. 

Robinson, J. T. 1956. The dentition of the Australopithecinae. Transvaal 
Museum (Pretoria) Memoir 9. 178 p. 

Tomes, C. S. 1923. A manual of dental anatomy, human and comparative, 
8th ed. J. and A. Churchill, London. 547 p. 

Washburn, S. L. 1960. Tools and human evolution. Sci. Am. 203: 3-15. 

Yurkstas, A. A., and W. H. Emerson. 1954. Study of tooth contact during 
mastication with artificial dentures. J. Pros. Dent. 4: 169-174. 

van Hooff, J. A. R. A. M. 1962. Facial expression in higher primates, p. 
97-125. In Evolutionary aspects of animal communication. Symp. Zool. 
Soc. Lond. 8. 

Zuckerman, S. 1958. Correlation of change in the evolution of higher 
primates, p. 300-352. In Huxley, Julian, A. C. Hardy, and E. B. Ford 
[eds.] Evolution as a process. George Allen and Unwin, London. 


REVIEW 


STYLE 


FORM 


TITLE 


ABSTRACT 


NOMENCLATURE 


ILLUSTRATIONS 


FOOTNOTES 


TABLES 


REFERENCES 


AUTHOR'S COPIES 


—— 


PROOF 


COPYRIGHT 


INFORMATION FOR AUTHORS 


The Publications Committee of the Peabody Museum of Natural 
History reviews and approves manuscripts for publication. Papers 
will be published in approximately the order in which they are 
accepted; delays may result if manuscript or illustrations are not in 
proper form. To facilitate review, the original and one carbon or 
xerox copy of the typescript and figures should be submitted. The 
author should keep a copy. 


Authors of biological papers should follow the Style Manual for 
Biological Journals, Second Edition (Amer. Inst. Biol. Sci.). Authors 
of paleontological manuscripts may choose to follow the Sugges- 
tions to Authors of the Reports of the U.S. Geological Survey, 
Fifth Edition (U.S. Govt. Printing Office). 


Maximum size is 80 printed pages including illustrations (= about 
100 manuscript pages including illustrations). Manuscripts must be 
typewritten, with wide margins, on one side of good quality 
842 x 11” paper. Double space everything. Do not underline any- 
thing except genera and species. The editors reserve the right to 
adjust style and form for conformity. 


Should be precise and short. Title should include pertinent key 
words which will facilitate computerized listings. Names of new 
taxa are not to be given in the title. 


The paper must begin with an abstract. Authors must submit com- 
pleted BioAbstract forms; these can be obtained from the Postilla 
editors in advance of submission of the manuscripts. 


Follow the International Codes of Zoological and Botanical Nomen- 
clature. 


Must be planned for reduction to 4 x 642” (to allow for running 
head and two-line caption). If illustration must go sideways on 
page, reduction should be to 334 x 634”. All illustrations should be 
called “Figures” and numbered in arabic, with letters for parts 
within one page. It is the author’s responsibility to see that illustra- 
tions are properly lettered and mounted. Captions should be typed 
double-spaced on a separate page. 


Should not be used, with rare exceptions. If unavoidable, type 
double-spaced on a separate page. 


Should be numbered in arabic. Each must be typed on a separate 
page. Horizontal rules should be drawn lightly in pencil; vertical 
rules must not be used. Tables are expensive to set and correct; 
cost may be lowered and errors prevented if author submits tables 
typed with electric typewriter for photographic reproduction. 


The style manuals mentioned above must be followed for form and 
for abbreviations of periodicals. Double space. 


Each author receives 50 free copies of his Postilla. Additional copies 
may be ordered at cost by author when he returns galley proof. 
All copies have covers. 

Author receives galley proof and manuscript for checking printer’s 
errors, but extensive revision cannot be made on the galley proof. 
Corrected galley proof and manuscript must be returned to editors 
within seven days. 

Any issue of Postilla will be copyrighted by Peabody Museum of 
Natural History only if its author specifically requests it. 


— 


OT 


3 2044 0 


neem TET UT Mee 
See seerecer te a a 
re Ba! wee WE > 
"ee heed 


~ ote ne ae} 
a ete, 


tet ae 4 
roajtainivakoit <a FWD 
ope 


oe nee a eee 
: Sees Shin, eeaan bane 


* 


Qe ardie « ; / 4 * 
: : “oe rows we 7 : 2 rfid o 
h f 2 pans"