Skip to main content

Full text of "Psychological monographs: general and applied"

See other formats


DC 


PSYCHOLOGICAL  REVIEW  PUBLICATIONS 


THE 

Psychological  Monographs 

* 

EDITED  BY 

JAMES  ROWLAND  ANGELL,  UNIVERSITY  OF  CHICAGO 
HOWARD  C  WARREN,  PRINCETON  UNIVERSITY  (Index) 

JOHN  B.  WATSON,  JOHNS  HOPKINS  UNIVERSITY  (Review)  and 
SHEPHERD  I.  FRANZ,  Govr.  HOSP.  FOR  INSANE  (Bulletin) 


VOLUME  XIX 

1915 


PSYCHOLOGICAL  REVIEW  COMPANY 

PRINCETON,  N.  J. 
AND  LANCASTER,  PA. 

AGENTS:    G.  E.  STECHERT  &  CO.,  LONDON  (2  Star  Yard,  Carey  St.,  W.  C.); 
LEIPZIG  (Koenig  Str.,  37) ;    PARIS  (16,  Rue  de  Cond6) 


TABLES  OF  CONTENTS  OF  VOL.  XIX 


i.  I.  Symptomatological  Differences  Associated  with  Similar 
Cerebral  Lesions  in  the  Insane.  II.  Variations  in  Distribu- 
tion of  the  Motor  Centers.  SHEPHERD  IVORY  FRANZ.  Pp. 
162. 


2.     The  Psycho-physiological  Effects  of  the  Elements  of  Speech  in 
Relation  to  Poetry.     ROBERT  C.  GIVLER.     Pp.  132. 


3.     Standardization    of    Tests    for    Defective    Children.     CLARA 
SCHMITT.     Pp.  181. 


4.     A  Study  of  Retroactive  Inhibition.  J.  EDGAR  DECAMP.  Pp.  69. 
(This  volume  includes  Monographs  81-84). 


M  XK  PSYCHOLOGICAL  REVIEW  PUBLICATIONS 

No.  1  Whole  No.  81 

THE 

Psychological  Monographs 

EDITED    BY 

JAMES  ROWLAND  ANGELL,  UNIVERSITY  OF  CHICAGO 
HOWARD  C.  WARREN,  PRINCETON  UNIVERSITY  (Index) 

JOHN  B.  WATSON,  JOHNS  HOPKINS  UNIVERSITY   (Review)   and 
SHEPHERD  I.  FRANZ,  GOVT.  HOSP.  FOR  INSANE  (Bulletin) 


ON  THE  FUNCTIONS  OF  THE  CEREBRUM 

I 

Symptomatological  Differences  As- 
sociated with  Similar  Cerebral 
Lesions  in  the  Insane 

By 
SHEPHERD   IVORY   FRANZ 

II 

Variations  in  Distribution  of  the 
Motor  Centers 

By 
SHEPHERD   IVORY   FRANZ 

With  the  Assistance  of 
J.    DUERSON    STOUT 


PSYCHOLOGICAL  REVIEW   COMPANY 

PRINCETON,   N.  J. 
AND   LANCASTER,  PA. 

AGENTS:   G.  E.  STECHERT  &  CO.,  LONDON  (2  Star  Yard,  Carey  St.,  W.  C.) : 
LEIPZIG  (Koenigstr.,  37):  PARIS  (16  rue  de  Conde) 


PREFACE 

The  two  articles  which  constitute  the  present  monograph  deal 
with  the  same  gen'eral  topic,  the  variations  in  function  of  cor- 
responding parts  of  different  brains.  This  matter  has  received 
scant  attention  in  neurological  literature,  notwithstanding  the 
fact  that  the  anatomical  variations  have  been  extensively  studied. 
The  data  recorded  in  the  two  articles  point  to  a  conclusion  which 
helps  to  an  understanding  and  to  a  conciliation  of  some  apparent 
discrepancies  in  previous  clinical  and  experimental  studies  of 
cerebral  function.  The  theoretical  discussion  which  is  given  is, 
however,  not  due  solely  to  the  work  now  presented,  but  in  great 
part  has  been  the  result  of  previous  personal  observations  and  of 
various  facts  which  have  been  recounted  in  clinico — and  physi- 
ological— neurological  literature. 

The  experimental  data  of  the  second  article  were  collected  be- 
fore the  examination  of  the  clinico-pathological  data  contained 
in  the  first  article  was  begun.  Many  results  of  the  experimental 
study  could  not  be  prepared  for  publication  in  the  present  article, 
and  a  number  of  duties  prevented  the  earlier  completion  of  the 
article  as  it  now  stands,  but  it  is  hoped  that  time  will  be  found 
for  the  early  presentation  of  the  other  collected  facts  which  bear 
upon  the  same  problem.  Part  of  the  first  study  was  prepared  for, 
but  was  not  presented  at,  the  conference  on  individual  differences 
at  Columbia  University  in  celebration  of  the  twenty-fifth  anniver- 
sary of  the  professorship  of  J.  McKeen  Cattell. 

In  the  experimental  part  of  the  present  work  the  author  has  had 
the  assistance  of  and  is  under  obligation  to  a  number  of  former 
students,  and  of  internes  at  the  Government  Hospital  for  the 
Insane,  too  numerous  to  mention  separately.  The  major  part  of 
the  assistance  was  given  by  Dr.  J.  Duerson  Stout,  now  associate 
professor  of  physiology  and  pharmacology  in  the  George  Wash- 
ington University  and  his  name  appears,  therefore,  on  the  title 
page. 


The  research  on  the  brains  of  the  monkeys  was  made  possible 
by  reason  of  a  grant  to  the  author,  for  the  purchase  and  main- 
tenance of  animals  for  the  investigation  of  the  functions  of  the 
cerebrum,  by  the  Carnegie  Institution  of  Washington,  and  for 
making  possible  this  and  other  similar  previous  investigations 
the  author  here  expresses  his  sense  of  obligation. 

For  the  convenience  of  the  reader  it  may  be  mentioned  that 
summaries  of  the  experimental  work  appear  at  the  ends  of  the 
individual  sections  of  that  article  (see  pp.  102,  105,  132,  and  139). 


CONTENTS 

PAGE 

Preface iii 

I.  SYMPTOMATOLOGICAL  DIFFERENCES  ASSO- 

CIATED WITH  SIMILAR  CEREBRAL  LESIONS 

IN  THE  INSANE. 

Introduction I 

Dementia  Precox,  clinical  histories 9 

Dementia  Precox,  discussion 19 

General  Paralysis  of  the  Insane,  clinical  histories 27 

General  Paralysis  of  the  Insane,  discussion 33 

Arteriosclerotic  Dementia,  clinical  histories 39 

Arteriosclerotic  Dementia,  discussion 45 

Senile  Dementia,  clinical  histories 52 

Senile  Dementia,  discussion 65 

Summary 74 

General  Discussion 77 

II.  VARIATIONS    IN    DISTRIBUTION    OF    THE 

MOTOR  CENTERS. 

Introduction 80 

Methods 82 

Experimental  Results 92 

I.  Extents  of  cerebral  motor  areas  for  the  arm  and  leg 

segments 95 

II.  Relative  distributions  of  areas  for  arm  and  leg  move- 
ments      103 

III.  Distributions  of  areas  for  individual  segments 106 

(Thigh,  Leg,  Foot,  Toes,  Shoulder,  Forearm,  Hand, 

Fingers,  General.) 

IV.  Anomalous  distributions  of  the  stimulable  areas. ...    134 
(Arm  movements  within  leg  areas;  Leg  movements 

within  arm  areas ;  Relatively  non-stimulable  zones. ) 

General  Discussion  (Theoretical) 14° 

References  .  


I 

SYMPTOMATOLOGICAL  DIFFERENCES 

ASSOCIATED    WITH    SIMILAR 

CEREBRAL  LESIONS  IN 

THE  INSANE 

INTRODUCTION 

It  is  well  known  that  in  different  diseases  similar  symptoms  are 
often  exhibited.  For  example,  anemia  and  fever  are  not  more 
characteristic  of  one  special  disease  than  of  a  multitude  of  others. 
On  the  other  hand,  discomfort  and  malaise  are  so  constantly  re- 
ported by  those  who  are  sick,  that  they  can  not  be  said  to  be  of 
more  than  minor  diagnostic  value.  Even  pains  are  so  commonly 
concomitants  of  different  diseases  that,  except  when  definitely 
localized,  and  not  always  then,  they  almost  cease  to  be  of  diagnos- 
tic or  prognostic  importance.  In  the  mental  disease,  the  individual 
symptom  is  often  less  valuable,  if  this  be  possible,  as  an  indication 
of  the  special  disease.  A  depression  or  a  sadness,  an  hallucination 
or  a  delusion,  defects  of  retention  or  of  comprehension,  and  evi- 
dences of  loose  thinking  and  the  like  are  elements  or  symptoms  in 
many  of  the  psychoses.  Not  one  is  pathognomonic  of  a  special 
disease,  each  is  an  expression  of  a  physiological  conflict  or  of  a 
loss  or  defect  of  anatomical,  and  also  functional,  cerebral  elements. 

Although  no  one  symptom  or  condition  can  be  relied  upon  for 
the  purpose  of  diagnosis,  the  combination  of  symptoms  does  give 
most  often  plain  evidence  of  the  nature  of  the  special  disorder. 
The  recognition  of  the  fact  that  diagnostic  reliance  may  be  placed 
upon  the  collection  or  concatenation  of  symptoms  has  led  to  the 
foundation  and  the  elaboration  of  a  system  of  knowledge,  we 
might  almost  say  a  science,  which  is  called  differential  diagnostics. 
The  development  of  this  field  has  also  been  due  to  the  realization 
that  in  the  same  disease  in  different  individuals  the  micro-or- 
ganisms may  produce  different  effects,  or  the  body  physiologically 


2  SHEPHERD  IVORY  FRANZ 

may  be  affected  differently,  and  there  may  result  different  prom- 
inent systems  in  different  individuals.  Apart  from  the  so-called 
mildness  or  the  severity  of  the  disease,  one  individual  may  ex- 
hibit high  temperature,  another  may  exhibit  a  temperature  only 
slightly  above  the  normal.  In  one  individual  the  diseased  con- 
dition of  one  of  the  heart  valves  may  exist  for  many  years  with- 
out obvious  symptoms  which  attract  the  patient's  attention  because 
of  compensation  in  both  the  strength  and  the  size  of  the  heart, 
while  a  similar  pathological  state  in  another  patient  gives  rise 
to  distress,  sharp  pains  and  faintness. 

These  similarities  of  symptoms  in  different  diseases  and  the 
variations  in  symptoms  in  different  individuals  with  the  same 
disease  are  paralleled  by  variations  in  actions  of  different  drugs 
and  by  differences  in  the  reactions  of  different  individuals  to 
some  foods.  In  some,  the  eating  of  strawberries  or  of  fish  is 
accompanied  by  disagreeable  effects,  and  in  other  individuals 
the  effects  following  the  administration  of  therapeutic  doses  of 
certain  drugs  are  not  only  disagreeable  but  often  dangerous. 
Small  amounts  of  the  derivatives  of  opium,  of  arsenic,  of  antipy- 
rine,  and  of  even  generally  supposed-to-be-harmless  quinine  at 
times  give  rise  to  violent  physiological  reaction,  although  most 
people  may  take  small  doses  of  these  drugs  with  impunity  and 
without  apparent  physiological  changes. 

In  the  older  psychiatry  individual  symptoms  or  concomitant 
physical  conditions  were  uncritically  believed  to  have  major 
importance  and,  because  of  this,  dissimilar  diseases  were  consider- 
ed to  be  the  same.  When  certain  etiological  factors  were  deter- 
mined and  when  the  symptomatological  variations  were  carefully 
considered  it  became  apparent  that  superficially  different  group- 
ings of  symptoms  might  be  and  often  are  essentially  similar. 
Notwithstanding  these  supposedly  fundamental  similarities  as 
we  see  them  at  present,  there  remain  many  prominent  points  of 
differences  in  the  symptoms  in  individuals  who  suffer  from  the 
same  disease.  These  individual  differences  have  been  supposed 
to  be  due  to  or  to  be  connected  with  variations  in  the  normal 
mental  make-up  of  the  patient,  or  to  variations  in  lesions  or  in 
functional  disturbances  of  cerebral  centers  or  connections. 


SYMPTOM ATOLOGICAL  DIFFERENCES  IN  THE  INSANE        3 

To  account  for  the  individual  variations  in  both  mental  and 
nervous  diseases,  the  psychoses  and  the  neuroses,  it  has  been 
most  easy  and  quite  satisfying  to  presuppose  functional  and 
structional  differences  of  the  lesions,  because  of  our  relative 
ignorance  of  many  of  the  functions  and  of  the  anatomical  con- 
nections of  parts  of  the  nervous  system.  This  is  specially  true 
when  we  deal  with  lesions  or  disease  of  the  cerebrum  and  the 
basal  ganglia.  Within  recent  years,  however,  there  has  been 
growing  the  realization  that  a  minute  lesion  in  one  part  of  the 
brain  may  give  rise  to  a  symptom  or  to  a  collection  of  symptoms 
which  is  exactly  the  same  as  that  resulting  from  a  similarly 
minute  lesion  in  a  second  or  a  third  portion  O'f  that  organ.  If 
we  liken  the  cerebral  mechanism  to  the  stations  and  lines  of  a 
telephone  or  a  telegraph  plant  we  may  readily  understand  how 
this  can  be.  If  the  emissive  element  or  the  transmitter  be  broken 
or  destroyed  it  is  not  possible  to  transmit  the  message  in  a  par- 
ticular direction  or  to  a  given  point.  The  function  is  abolished. 
But  we  must  also  keep  in  mind  that  a  similar  abnormal  condition 
of  function  arises  if,  instead  of  having  a  lesion  of  the  emissive 
element,  there  be  a  break  in  any  part  of  the  conducting  line. 
Such  an  injury  or  a  disease  may  be  close  to  or  far  from  the 
transmitter  and  may  even  be  in  the  receiver. 

Lesions  in  the  nervous  system  minute  enough  to  embrace  an 
individual  nerve  cell  or  its  processes  are  never  found.  The 
smallest  always  involve  many  elements.  But  even  though  this 
be  true,  it  has  been  shown  that  such  lesions  in  different  parts  may 
result  in  similar  physiological  disturbances.  Lesions  of  such 
widely  separated  parts  of  the  nervous  system  as  the  frontal 
lobes  and  the  cerebellum  are  known  to  produce  similar  symp- 
toms, and  in  many  cases  a  definite  diagnosis  can  be  made  only 
after  death.  Usually,  however,  concurrent  with  the  main,  or 
with  the  more  prominent,  symptoms  other  symptoms  are  found. 
These  additional  symptoms  point  to  disturbances  of  structures 
anatomically  allied  to  the  one  which  is  chiefly  involved  or  which 
is  entirely  destroyed,  and  they  permit  fairly  accurate  neurological 
diagnostic  localizations  in  many  cases  during  life.  Thus,  for 


4  SHEPHERD  IVORY  FRANZ 

example,  we  find  it  possible  to  predict  the  localizations  of  lesions 
in  individuals  with  paralyses  because  of  the  association  of  several 
paralyzed  segments,  or  because  of  concomitant  sensibility  dis- 
orders. 

The  reverse  state  of  affairs  is  seldom  considered.  Is  it,  we  may 
ask,  possible  that  anatomically  similar  lesions  of  the  cerebrum 
give  rise  to  dissimilar  symptoms  in  different  individuals?  The 
consequences  of  a  positive  or  a  negative  answer  to  this  question 
are  of  great  importance.  If  similar  cerebral  lesions  do  not  al- 
ways produce  similar  symptoms,  there  is  opened  a  series  of 
problems  regarding  the  "why"  and  the  "how"  of  cerebral  func- 
tion which  are  fundamental.  If  similar  cerebral  lesions  are 
always  accompanied  by  similar  mental  changes,  our  conceptions 
of  cerebral  mechanics  may  remain  simple  and  our  explanations 
of  the  relations  of  mental  and  cerebral  functions  become  less 
difficult. 

Because  of  these  considerations  it  appeared  desirable  to  make 
a  special  study  of  possible  differences  in  symptoms  accompanying 
similar  cerebral  lesions.  For  this  purpose  there  were  available 
autopsy  and  clinical  records  of  nearly  3,300  patients  who  had 
been  in  the  Government  Hospital  for  the  Insane.  The  clinical 
records  of  many  were  so  meagre  that  attempted  correlations  of 
the  cerebral  lesions  with  the  clinical  symptoms  would,  in  these 
cases,  have  been  futile,  and  it  appeared  would  have  resulted  in 
a  loss  of  time.  For  this  reason  only  those  cases  have  been 
considered  in  which  there  were  recorded  the  results  of  fairly 
complete  mental  examinations  as  well  as  the  autopsy  examina- 
tions. These  considerations  resulted  in  the  primary  rejections 
of  all  but  the  last  950  autopsy  cases,  representing  roughly  those 
autopsies  which  had  been  performed  during  the  past  six  or  seven 
years.  It  was  also  found  that  about  one-third  of  these  must 
also  be  rejected  on  account  of  insufficient  clinical  examinations, 
and  because  of  indefinite  diagnostic  data.  The  cases  which  were 
eventually  selected  were  of  all  kinds  of  mental  diseases,  but  the 
series  is  particularly  strong  in  the  organic  psychoses,  such  as 
paresis,  arteriosclerotic  dementia,  senile  dementia,  and  the  like. 


SYMPTOMATOLOGICAL  DIFFERENCES  IN  THE  INSANE        5 

The  brains  of  these  individuals  showed  a  great  variety  of 
lesions,  inflammatory,  atrophic,  hemorrhagic,  and  the  like.  These 
were  divided  into  two  general  groups,  one  in  which  there  was  a 
single  or  unitary  lesion,  the  other  in  which  there  was  a  com- 
bination of  cerebral  lesions.  Those  in  which  there  were  two 
kinds  of  lesions,  e.  g.,  softening  and  atrophy,  were  excluded. 
The  cases  which  showed  atrophy  were  the  most  numerous  and 
these  were  selected  for  the  present  study.  These  were  separated 
into  two  general  classes:  (i)  Those  in  which  the  atrophy  was 
general  but  in  which  there  might  be  a  greater  atrophic  condition 
in  one  portion  of  the  cerebrum, and  (2)  those  in  which  the  atrophy 
was  well  localized  in  one  special  region  of  the  cerebral  cortex. 
The  second  group  is  the  one  which  has  been  more  carefully  stud- 
ied at  the  present  time.  From  this  group,  as  has  been  stated 
above,  there  have  been  omitted  almost  all  those  cases  in  which 
there  were  other  gross  lesions  of  parts  of  the  cerebrum  or  of  the 
nervous  system  in  general.  A  few  cases  in  which  the  only  ad- 
ditional lesions  were  recent  cerebral  hemorrhages,  that  resulted 
in  the  death  of  the  patients,  were  included,  because  whatever 
mental  changes  had  been  observed  during  the  major  part  of 
their  hospital  residence  could  not  have  been  due  to  these  lesions. 
It  might  also  have  been  possible  to  include  certain  cases  in  which 
localized  softenings  accompanied  the  atrophy,  because  in  a  num- 
ber of  cases  these  additional  pathological  conditions  were  due  to 
comparatively  recent  cerebral  insults.  Since,  however,  definite 
dates  could  not  be  assigned  to  some  of  them,  they  have  been 
excluded  from  the  present  report. 

The  atrophies  which  are  dealt  with  here  are  those  which,  as 
defined  by  Blackburn1,  imply  "reduction  in  size  and  weight  of 
an  organ  which  has  been  at  one  time  of  greater  volume  and  weight, 
though  the  organ  may  not  have  been  orginally  up  to  the  normal 
standard.  It  also  implies  that  this  atrophy  is  the  result  of  de- 
generation and  diminution  of  the  elements  of  the  tissue  and  not 
merely  the  result  of  pressure  or  gross  loss  of  substance."  In 
old  age  this  condition  of  atrophy  of  the  cerebrum  is  met  with  as 

1  Blackburn,  I.  W.    Atrophy  of  the  Brain  in  the  Insane.    Govt.  Hasp,  for 
the  Insane  Bull.,  1911,  3,  45-50. 


6  SHEPHERD  IVORY  FRANZ 

a  common  concomitant  of  the  general  bodily  degenerative 
changes,  and  it  may  be  due  solely  to  these  degenerative  tend- 
encies of  the  body  as  a  whole.  The  condition  of  cerebral  atrophy 
is  also  to  be  found  in  a  variety  of  other  mental  diseases  besides 
that  of  senile  dementia,  and  it  is  particularly  noticeable  in  many 
cases  of  general  paralysis  of  the  insane.  The  atrophies  may,  it 
has  been  shown,  be  primary,  i.e.,  due  to  degenerations  of  the 
cells,  without  concomitant  changes  in  the  blood  vessels,  or  they 
may  be  the  result  of  partial  blocking  of  or  difficulties  in  the 
course  of  the  blood  flow.  The  atrophies  which  accompany  cerebral 
arteriosclerosis  are  allied,  some  think,  to  the  atrophies  which  are 
found  in  general  paralysis  of  the  insane,  in  that  they  are  atrophies 
of  a  secondary  nature,  whereas  the  atrophies  which  are  found  in 
senile  dementia,  and  possibly  also  in  dementia  precox,  are  more 
frequently  primary  atrophies.  In  many  cases  the  atrophies  are  so 
distributed  that  it  is  apparent  they  do  not  depend  upon  changes 
in  the  arterial  supply.  In  fact,  in  many  instances  the  gross  and 
also  the  histological  examinations  may  .fail  to  show  any  sclerotic 
changes  in  the  blood  vessels.  In  this  connection,  it  may  be  noted 
that  Blackburn  has  pointed  out  that  in  certain  cases  there  may 
be  a  complete  preservation  of  certain  cortical  areas  with  an 
atrophy  of  surrounding  or  neighboring  zones  which  are  sup- 
plied by  the  same  vessel. 

In  many  cases  the  cerebral  atrophy  is  general,  but  regional  atro- 
phies are  frequently  met  with,  such  as  those  of  the  frontal,  or 
parietal  or  temporal  lobes.  In  paresis  it  is  not  uncommon  to 
find  such  localized  atrophies,  and  these  are  more  frequent  in 
the  frontal  lobes.  There  is,  however,  an  atrophy  of  the  parietal 
region,  circumscribed  in  character,  which  has  been  described  by 
Lissauer.  Although  the  symptoms  due  to  this  circumscribed 
atrophic  condition  of  the  posterior  half  of  the  brain  include  cer- 
tain focal  manifestations,  especially  those  of  disturbances  in 
speech  of  the  nature  of  the  aphasias,  the  other  symptoms  do 
not  differ  very  markedly  from  the  symptoms  accompanying  more 
generalized  or  frontal  atrophy,  and  previous  to  death  it  is  not 
usually  possible  to  diganose  this  condition.  Kraepelin2  reports 

1  Kraepelin,  E.,  General  Paresis  (trans,  by  I.  W.  Moore).  Nerv.  and 
Ment.  Dis.  Monog.  No.  14.  Pp.  v  -f-  200.  (See  especially  pp.  134-135.) 


SYMPTOMATOLOGICAL  DIFFERENCES  IN  THE  INSANE        7 

that  in  this  Lissauer  type  of  paresis  the  course  of  the  disease  is 
spasmodic  and  resembles,  to  some  extent,  cortical  epilepsy,  while 
the  deterioration  is  more  gradual  than  in  the  frontal  atrophies. 
This  type  of  case  is  estimated  by  Alzheimer  to  comprise  about 
15  per  cent  of  the  total  cases  of  paresis.  Similar  circumscribed, 
or  regional,  atrophies  are  also  found  in  cases  of  senile  dementia, 
arteriosclerotic  dementia,  and  dementia  precox. 

In  regard  to  the  relation  of  atrophies  to  the  clinical  symptoms 
Blackburn  has  written :  "The  conclusions  reached  by  long  ex- 
perience are  that  in  all  cases  of  insanity  of  long  standing  in  which 
there  is  a  demonstrable  mental  deterioration  we  may  confidently 
predict  that  some  shrinkage  of  the  brain  may  be  found;  that  as 
a  rule  the  degree  of  dementia  is  commensurate  with  the  atrophy 
found  or  present;  and  that  the  localization  of  this  shrinkage  in 
the  prefrontal  region  in  most  cases  is  a  strong  presumptive  evi- 
dence of  the  seat  of  intellectual  processes  in  that  part  of  the 
brain."3  He  furthermore  states  that  the  fact  that  the  secondary 
degenerative  atrophy  "is  confined  most  frequently  to  the  frontal 
lobes  and  the  prefrontal  region  is  strikingly  significant  in  view 
of  the  supposed  intellectual  function  of  these  parts."  On  the 
other  hand,  it  should  be  mentioned  that  senile  dementia  may 
be  evident  without  concomitant  atrophic  conditions  in  the  brain. 
This  is  also  true  for  dementia  precox,  and  whatever  relations 
the  atrophic  conditions  bear  to  the  changes  in  mental  characters 
which  are  grouped  together  under  the  general  term  "mental 
deterioration"  are  at  present  unknown.  Since  deterioration 
may  exist  without  obvious  atrophy  it  is  apparent  that  the 
atrophy,  in  itself,  is  not  necessary  for  the  production  of  the 
symptoms.  That,  however,  the  symptoms  do,  in  many  cases 
at  least,  depend  upon  the  cerebral  changes  we  may  believe.  The 
relation,  direct  or  indirect,  of  the  cerebral  lesions  with  the 
symptoms  has  not  yet  been  sufficiently  shown. 

After  the  elimination   of   the  cases  with  multiple  lesions   a 

total  of  sixty  cases  was  obtained  with  sufficiently  complete  clinical 

and  pathological  histories  to  make  certain  comparisons  of  value. 

Some  of  these  were  not  usable  in  the  present  study  on  account 

•  Op.  fit. 


8  SHEPHERD  IVORY  FRANZ 

of  the  fact  that  they  were  diagnostically  doubtful  cases  or 
there  were  too  few  cases  of  the  special  kind  of  mental  disease 
to  make  valuable  symptom-lesion  correlations.  In  general  it 
was  believed  that  at  least  four  or  more  cases  of  a  particular 
psychosis  were  needed  if  the  individual  differences  and  similari- 
ties were  to  be  dealt  with  properly,  and  on  account  of  this 
twenty-two  cases  were  omitted  from  the  present  work.  The 
omitted  cases  were :  manic-depressive  and  allied  psychoses,  7 ;  in- 
toxication psychoses,  2 ;  imbecility,  I ;  epilepsy  with  dementia,  i ; 
paralysis  agitans,  i ;  cerebral  syphilis,  3 ;  organic  diseases  of  the 
brain  not  otherwise  differentiated,  3 ;  undiagnosed  or  unclassified 
psychoses,  4.  The  remaining  thirty-eight  cases  were  distributed 
as  follows:  dementia  precox,  9;  general  paralysis  of  the  insane, 
6;  arteriosclerotic  dementia,  9;  senile  dementia,  14.  Thirteen 
of  these  cases  were  described  in  the  autopsy  records  as  simple 
frontal  atrophies ;  two  were  cases  in  which  the  brain  was  generally 
atrophied  but  the  atrophies  of  the  frontal  regions  were  great; 
eighteen  cases  were  described  as  anterior  atrophies,  and  by  this  is 
meant  that  the  regions  anterior  to  the  central  fissure  (including, 
therefore,  the  so-called  motor  region  in  addition  to  the  'frontal 
area)  were  atrophied ;  .four  cases  showed  atrophy  over  the  anter- 
ior two-thirds  of  the  cerebrum,  including  therefore  more  of  the 
cortex  than  in  the  cases  previously  mentioned;  one  additional 
case,  in  which  the  atrophy  covered  the  frontal  region  and  the 
posterior  portion  of  the  parietal  area,  was  included  'for  com- 
parison. The  distribution  of  these  extents  of  atrophies  in 
the  different  diseases  is  given  in  the  accompanying  table  and 
comparisons  are  also  made  in  the  discussions. 


Mental  Diseases 

Characters  of  atrophy 

Frontal 

Frontal 
and 
general 

Anterior 

Anterior 
two-thirds 

Frontal 
and  pos- 
terior 
parietal 

Totals 

Dementia  precox  .... 
General  paralysis  of  the 
insane  

3 

3 

2 

5 

o 

0 

2 
0 

5 

3 
5 
5 

I 

0 
0 

3 

0 
0 

o 
i 

9 

6 
9 
M 

Arteriosclerotic  dementia 
Senile  dementia    .... 

Totals 

13 

2 

18 

4 

i 

38 

DEMENTIA  PRECOX,  CLINICAL  HISTORIES 

Case  i,  white  female,  was  admitted  to  the  Hospital  at  the 
age  of  54  and  lived  for  3  years  and  6  months. 

Her  family  history  was  bad;  her  father  was  nervous,  her 
maternal  cousin  was  insane,  and  her  mother  was  an  invalid  for  a 
number  of  years  before  her  death,  which  was  due  to  ovarian  tu- 
mor; the  patient  attended  private  schools  until  the  age  of  seven- 
teen ;  she  was  considered  to  be  sickly  all  her  life ;  for  many  years 
she  showed  marked  peculiarities  of  conduct,  was  contrary,  and  had 
spells  of  high  temper  at  intervals  varying  from  a  week  to  several 
months;  at  one  time  she  lived  in  an  eighteen-room  house  with 
only  a  dog  for  company  for  a  period  of  fifteen  years.  The  mental 
disorder  which  led  to  her  commitment  was  probably  of  very  long 
standing,  but  the  occasion  of  commitment  was  the  manifesta- 
tion of  delusions  of  persecution  by  "witches"  and  by  the  "Black 
Hand  Society" ;  she  was  also  extremely  nervous,  had  insomia, 
refused  to  eat,  and  exhibited  homicidal  tendencies.  She  would 
not  permit  either  physical  or  neurological  examination,  but  ex- 
hibited no  obvious  physical  or  neurological  abnormalities  except 
a  few  small  sores  over  trunk  and  limbs.  She  was  restless,  suspi- 
cious of  almost  every  one  about  her;  she  refused  to  talk  freely 
about  herself;  at  times  she  imagined  the  food  was  poisoned  and 
refused  to  take  it  on  this  account,  but  on  being  assured  that 
there  was  no  poison  or  after  it  had  been  tasted  by  another,  she 
would  take  it;  she  also  feared  that  the  towels,  the  combs,  the 
water,  etc.,  contained  poison;  she  had  hallucinations  of  hearing 
(she  had  heard  her  mother's  and  brother's  voices  talking  to  her) 
and  also  of  skin  sensations  (she  said  that  she  was  electrical  and 
could  give  out  power  at  any  time ;  she  also  reported  that  electric 
currents  had  been  turned  upon  her  for  years  by  her  enemies)  ;  she 
believed  that  she  was  especially  favored  by  the  Lord  and  that  she 
received  signs  of  this;  her  memory  was  excellent  and  when  she 
was  persuaded  to  talk,  she  gave  detailed  accounts  of  her  past  and 
of  the  conditions  which  led  up  to  the  persecutions  to  which  she  be- 
lieved she  had  been  subjected ;  her  attention  did  not  appear  to  be 


io  SHEPHERD  IVORY  FRANZ 

impaired,  and  there  was  no  distractability ;  she  had  no  apprecia- 
tion of  her  mental  condition ;  she  was  well  oriented  in  all  spheres ; 
there  was  no  clouding  of  consciousness;  she  reported  (in- 
correctly) that  she  did  not  sleep  well;  and  she  answered  in- 
coherently at  most  times.  Later  she  became  indifferent,  but  at 
times  was  noted  to  be  emotionally  labile;  she  was  irresponsible; 
her  answers  were  circumstantial,  irrelevant,  or  incoherent;  at 
times  she  was  disturbed,  noisy,  destructive,  violent,  and  untidy; 
she  wandered  away ;  talked  constantly ;  her  answers  showed  that 
her  memory  was  poor  for  recent  events,  and  there  was  evidence 
of  gradual  mental  deterioration. 

Death  was  due  to  purulent  parotitis  and  bronchopneumonia. 
Besides  these,  the  autopsy  revealed :  diffuse  nephritis ;  pulmonary 
tubercular  nodules;  an  insufficiency  of  the  tricuspid  valve  of  the 
heart ;  numerous  uterine  fibroids  and  endometritis ;  the  brain  was 
slightly  shrunken  in  the  frontal  regions,  but  no  other  gross  cere- 
bral lesions  were  found.  The  histological  examination  showed  a 
slight  chromatolysis  of  the  ganglion  cells,  neuronophagia,  and  an 
increase  of  the  neuroglia. 

Case  2,  white  male,  36  years  of  age  at  the  time  of  admission, 
lived  25  years  and  a  half  in  the  Hospital.  The  duration  of  the 
mental  disease  at  the  time  of  admission  was  noted  as  2  years 
and  3  months. 

His  family  history  was  negative;  the  patient  came  to  the 
United  States  when  young,  entered  the  army,  and,  as  has  been 
indicated  above,  exhibited  for  more  than  two  years  previous  to 
his  entrance  to  the  Hospital  mental  abnormalities.  Physically 
and  neurologically  no  pathological  conditions  were  detected. 
He  was  quiet,  tidy,  had  little  to  say  voluntarily,  but  would  answer 
questions;  his  answers  showed  that  he  had  delusions  of  a  fan- 
tastic nature  which  were  fleeting  in  character,  but  which  did  not, 
however,  appear  to  cause  him  any  discomfort ;  he  appeared  to  be 
indifferent  to  his  surroundings,  but  in  general  was  satisfied  and 
happy,  industrious,  and  he  willingly  helped  with  the  ward  work; 
his  memory  was  fair;  in  general  he  was  disoriented;  his  delu- 
sions concerned  God  and  religion,  and  combined  with  these,  there 


SYMPTOM ATOLOGICAL  DIFFERENCES  IN  THE  INSANE      n 

were  auditory  hallucinations  that  the  "Almighty"  talked  with 
him  and  that  other  voices  abused  him,  and  for  that  reason  he 
swore  at  them  occasionally;  he  often  talked  to  himself.  Later 
he  became  almost  completely  disoriented;  memory  became  very 
poor,  for  the  most  part  he  talked  unintelligibly,  but  he  gave 
plain  evidence  of  the  existence  of  delusions;  he  said  that  God 
Almightly  persecuted  him  by  ordering  him  to  do  things  he  did  not 
like  to  do;  he  occasionally  stopped  eating  in  the  midst  of  a  meal 
and  explained  this  as  being  due  to  the  fact  that  God  had  told 
him  to  eat  no  more ;  he  used  very  profane  language  and  swore  at 
times,  and  would  have  nothing  to  do  with  the  other  patients;  he 
asked  for  whiskey  to  drink  so  that  in  this  way  he  might  punish 
God  Almighty;  he  kissed  the  walls;  he  muttered  unintelligibly, 
and  he  informed  a  physician  that  God's  voice  was  inside  his 
body,  and  that  God  talked  so  quickly  that  he  could  not  repeat 
what  was  said ;  he  showed  no  interest  in  his  surroundings  except 
as  exhibited  in  his  daily,  almost  continuous,  polishing  of  the 
floor,  which  he  said  was  done  at  the  command  of  God;  he  had 
practically  no  knowledge  of  current  events;  his  reasoning  and 
judgment  were  very  much  impaired;  he  was  illogical;  his  con- 
duct was  silly ;  and  his  insight  was  almost  lacking. 

At  the  autopsy  there  were  found:  marked  generalized  ar- 
teriosclerosis; calcification  of  the  mitral  valves  of  the  heart; 
hypostatic  congestion  of  the  lungs;  interstitial  nephritis;  and 
shrinkage  of  the  frontal  portions  of  the  cerebrum. 

Case  3,  colored  male,  exhibited  mental  abnormalities  for  5 
or  more  years  previous  to  his  admission  at  the  age  of  30.  Hos- 
pital residence  was  1 1  years  and  6  months. 

Nothing  was  learned  of  this  patient's  family  or  previous  per- 
sonal history  except  that  it  was  reported  he  had  been  mentally 
abnormal  for  at  least  five  years  previous  to  his  admission,  although 
his  marked  mental  abnormality  was  evident  for  only  a  year 
previous  to  his  commitment ;  during  that  time  he  was  noted  to  be 
melancholy  and  restless;  he  showed  extreme  anxiety  and  had 
hallucinations;  he  upset  everything  in  his  room,  and  was  untidy 
and  filthy  in  habits.  Physical  and  neurological  examinations 


12  SHEPHERD  IVORY  FRANZ 

were  negative  except  that  hearing  and  smell  were  slightly  im- 
paired; his  gait  was  slow  and  his  attitude,  although  fairly  erect 
and  steady,  was  slightly  relaxed;  his  actions  in  general  were 
slow,  imperfect,  uncertain  and  unreliable.  He  was  quiet,  dull, 
often  stupid;  he  was  also  untidy;  memory  was  markedly  im- 
paired; his  ideation  was  slow  and  uncertain;  in  general  he  ans- 
wered fairly  well,  although  at  times  irrelevantly;  his  reasoning 
and  judgment  were  bad;  he  appeared  to  have  very  little  mental 
capacity ;  no  hallucinations  or  delusions  were  observed  and  could 
not  be  detected  from  his  actions ;  he  was  unappreciative  and  had 
no  apparent  interest  in  his  surroundings;  he  appeared  stupid;  he 
usually  sat  in  one  place  and  assumed  one  position ;  he  spoke  only 
when  spoken  to,  and  then  only  after  much  persuasion;  at  times 
he  was  noisy,  turbulent,  talked  incoherently,  and  was  destructive ; 
echopraxia,  mutism,  negativism  and  catatonia  were  observed,  but 
none  of  these  conditions  was  marked ;  orientation  was  lacking. 

The  patient  died  of  tubercular  pneumonia,  and  shrinkage  of 
the  frontal  lobes  of  the  cerebrum  was  also  found  at  the  autopsy. 

Case  4,  white  male,  entered  the  Hospital  at  the  age  of  54  and 
lived  :for  18  years. 

He  fought  in  the  Civil  War  and  since  its  ending  (he  was  twenty- 
two  years  old  at  that  time)  was  abnormal,  with  intervening  lucid 
intervals ;  the  character  of  his  abnormality  during  this  period  of 
twenty-one  years  was  not  very  clear,  but  presumably  it  was  of  a 
precox  nature.  On  his  entrance  to  the  Hospital  he  was  untidy ;  he 
answered  irrelevantly;  he  exhibited  poor  memory;  he  said  little; 
he  went  out  walking  daily ;  at  times  he  would  not  keep  his  cloth- 
ing on  and  was  filthy  in  habits.  Later  the  patient  was  found  to 
have  interstitial  nephritis,  and  ,for  about  seven  years  before  his 
death  he  was  almost  entirely  confined  to  bed  on  account  of  this 
pathological  condition  and  heart  disease;  he  became  much  de- 
mented, took  no  interest  in  his  surroundings ;  questions  often  re- 
mained unanswered,  but  if  answers  were  obtained,  they  showed 
no  grasp  of  his  surroundings  and  were  given  in  a  slow,  hesitating 
manner  and  in  so  low  a  voice  as  to  be  almost  inaudible  and  unin- 
telligible; he  talked  to  himself  a  great  deal  and  appeared  to  have 


SYMPTOMATOLOGICAL  DIFFERENCES  IN  THE  INSANE      13 

auditory  hallucinations;  occasionally  he  arose  from  his  bed  and 
peered  about  the  ward;  echolalia  was  present;  he  remained  most 
of  the  time  in  bed  motionless  with  his  arms  across  his  chest  and 
his  fingers  intertwined. 

Death  was  due  to  purpura  hemorrhagica.  The  autopsy  showed : 
some  heart  disease ;  atheromatous  aorta ;  small  cysts  in  the  kidney ; 
hypernephroma ;  chronic  cytitis ;  the  section  of  the  brain  revealed 
no  lesions  except  atrophy  of  the  anterior  convolutions. 

Case  5,  colored  male,  lived  for  about  2  years  after  admission 
at  the  age  of  23. 

A  brother  and  sister  had  spasms  in  early  life;  otherwise  the 
family  history  was  negative ;  he  attended  school  for  five  years,  but 
made  little  progress;  he  drank  alcoholic  liquors  to  excess  and 
sometimes  was  drunk  two  or  three  times  a  week;  he  had  gonor- 
rhea several  times  and  a  chancre  a  year  previous  to  his  admis- 
sion; he  was  arrested  on  account  of  a  brawl  and  sent  to  the 
workhouse,  where  his  mental  condition  was  recognized  as  ab- 
normal. From  there  he  was  sent  to  the  Hospital,  where  he  was 
excited;  he  said  that  another  inmate  had  shot  some  dope  or 
chloroform  into  him,  that  all  the  other  prisoners  were  dodging 
about  to  avoid  it,  this  was  blown  at  him  through  a  long  stem, 
and  he  could  not  sleep  and  felt  queer ;  he  believed  he  would  have 
been  killed  by  it  if  he  had  remained,  for  he  heard  them  talking 
about  "kill  that  nigger,"  and  he  became  frightened  and  excited; 
bells  were  also  rung  in  his  ears,  voices  asked  him  how  he  felt; 
he  believed  some  one  was  after  him  trying  to  read  his  mind;  he 
said  that  the  poisoned  stuff  which  was  being  shot  at  him  he 
could  feel,  smell,  and  taste,  but  could  not  see  the  dust  of  it,  and 
could  not  see  the  people;  some  nights  he  jumped  out  of  bed 
because  he  thought  electricity  was  being  used  upon  him  and  for 
several  days  he  had  the  feeling  of  things  crawling  over  his  body 
like  insects.  The  physical  and  neurological  examinations  re- 
vealed no  abnormalities  except  a  complete  positive  Wassermann 
of  the  blood  serum ;  he  was  uneducated,  and  his  general  informa- 
tion and  memory  were  poor,  but  he  exhibited  a  good  memory 
for  occurrences  in  his  own  life;  at  first  he  was  quiet  and  orderly; 


I4  SHEPHERD  IVORY  FRANZ 

he  was  tidy  in  habits;  he  helped  with  the  ward  work;  he  ap- 
preciated his  surroundings;  he  answered  questions  promptly,  but 
talked  little  with  the  other  patients ;  and  he  appeared  to  be  some- 
what depressed.  Later  he  became  sullen;  he  stood  in  various 
places  about  the  ward;  he  was  slow  in  movements  and  showed 
a  tendency  to  remain  in  one  attitude  for  a  considerable  length 
of  time;  he  took  no  interest  in  what  went  on  about  him;  he 
never  spoke  unless  spoken  to;  he  was  disoriented  for  time  but 
oriented  for  place  and  persons;  he  had  auditory  hallucinations 
of  voices  which  seemed  to  come  'from  his  stomach,  and  which 
were  interpreted  as  the  spirit  of  God  talking  to  him;  the  voices 
said  different  things,  but  he  could  not,  or  would  not,  recount  any 
particular  thing;  insight  was  lacking;  pulmonary  tuberculosis 
was  diagnosed  three  months  before  death. 

The  autopsy  showed:  pulmonary  and  intestinal  tuberculosis; 
fibrous  deposits  over  the  small  intestine,  and  infiltration  of  the 
mesenteric  glands;  slight  shrinkage  of  the  anterior  portions  of 
the  cerebrum,  but  no  other  gross  cerebral  lesions  on  section. 

Case  6,  white  male,  was  25  years  old  at  the  time  of  admission 
and  lived  28  years  subsequently. 

No  family  or  previous  personal  history  was  obtained  except 
that  the  patient  exhibited  mental  abnormalities  for  a  month  pre- 
vious to  his  admission ;  during  his  Hospital  residence  he  became 
gradually  demented  and  exhibited  periodic  variations  in  his 
behavior.  At  one  time  he  would  be  quiet  and  orderly,  and  he 
would  sit  or  stand  for  hours  at  a  time  in  one  place ;  for  the  most 
part  he  was  tidy  in  his  habits;  he  seldom  answered  and  volun- 
teered no  information;  he  appeared  to  be  dull  and  stupid  and  in 
almost  a  semi-conscious  condition.  This  state  would  last  for  a 
week  or  two  and  be  replaced  by  one  of  general  activity ;  he  sang 
loudly  and  irrationally  nearly  all  night;  he  was  restless,  destruc- 
tive, and  untidy  in  dress  and  habits;  he  ran  around  the  ward 
moving  or  picking  up  everything  he  could  reach.  At  times  his 
face  was  expressionless  and  at  other  times  he  appeared  to  be 
very  much  depressed ;  his  talk  was  unintelligible,  and  he  mumbled 
to  himself  continually;  it  was  judged  that  he  had  auditory  hal- 


SYMPTOMATOLOGICAL  DIFFERENCES  IN  THE  INSANE      15 

lucinations  because  at  times  he  turned  his  head  suddenly  to  the 
right  or  left  and  muttered  unintelligibly  or  excitably  as  if  he 
were  talking  to  or  scolding  some  one;  some  of  his  mutterings 
which  were  heard  indicated  that  he  believed  some  one  was  after 
him  and  wished  to  harm  him;  he  showed  no  interest  in  his  sur- 
roundings ;  he  could  not  be  made  to  work ;  his  memory  was  poor. 
In  his  later  years  it  was  noted  that  he  "exhibited  no  marks  of 
intelligence";  he  either  did  not  understand  questions  or  could 
not  make  himself  understood ;  his  clothing  was  disheveled ;  he  was 
filthy  in  habits;  and  when  he  could  he  stole  from  his  fellow 
patients ;  the  only  reaction  which  was  often  elicited  when  he  was 
spoken  to  was  the  opening  and  closing  of  his  eyes. 

Death  was  due  to  pulmonary  tuberculosis ;  in  addition  to  this 
condition,  cardiac  atrophy  and  atrophy  of  the  anterior  portions 
of  the  cerebrum  were  noted  at  the  autopsy. 

Case  7,  white  male,  36  years  of  age  at  the  time  of  admission; 
had  exhibited  mental  abnormalities  for  4  or  more  years  previous 
to  his  admission,  and  lived  in  the  Hospital  for  7  years. 

This  patient  was  a  wife  murderer  who  showed  the  following 
evidence  of  insanity  immediately  after  his  imprisonment  for  life; 
he  was  melancholic;  he  was  unconscious  of  his  surroundings; 
he  answered  in  monosyllables  when  at  all;  part  of  the  time  he 
would  not  talk  and  he  was  noted  to  have  a  treacherous  disposition. 
Whether  or  not  the  murder  of  his  wife,  for  which  he  was  con- 
victed, was  due  to  paranoid  ideas  was  not  determined,  but  in  view 
of  his  later  history  this  seems  probable,  and  the  psychosis  may 
have  begun  many  years  previous  to  the  date  assigned  above. 
His  expression  was  dull  and  indifferent ;  his  attitude  was  stooped 
and  careless,  and  he  walked  in  an  apparently  reckless  and  slovenly 
manner.  Physical  and  neurological  examinations  showed  no 
abnormalities  of  importance.  On  admission  he  recognized  what 
was  said  to  him;  he  knew  where  he  was;  he  exhibited  a  fair 
memory ;  he  rubbed  his  hair  and  face,  twisted  his  mouth,  grinned 
meaninglessly  and  hummed  to  himself;  he  attempted  to  strike 
an  attendant  with  a  shoe ;  but  at  this  time  no  delusions  or  halluci- 
nations were  detected;  he  was  restless,  continually  moved  about 


16  SHEPHERD  IVORY  FRANZ 

the  ward,  he  walked  rapidly  and  in  an  excited  manner ;  he  wanted 
his  own  way,  but  otherwise  was  apparently  indifferent  to  what 
went  on;  he  rarely  spoke  voluntarily,  but  sometimes  sang  to 
himself ;  he  was  noisy  at  times  and  at  other  times  appeared  to  be 
depressed ;  he  was  clean  in  habits ;  he  butted  his  head  against  the 
door  and  walls  of  his  room;  false  hearing  was  suspected  on  ac- 
count of  his  frequent  talking  to  himself;  he  used  abusive  terms 
towards  parts  of  the  room  in  which  no  one  was  present;  he 
would  not  tell  what  or  who  was  bothering  him;  he  shouted  vile 
names ;  he  answered  general  questions,  although  this  was  done  in 
a  surly  manner.  Later  his  hallucinations  and  delusions  became 
more  evident;  he  believed  that  a  woman  was  after  him,  that  she 
came  into  his  room  at  night  to  bother  him ;  he  called  to  her  out 
of  the  window,  using  a  number  of  names  indicating  her  indecent 
character;  he  also  bawled  at  some  chickens  which  he  believed 
were  on  the  floor  under  his  feet,  and  he  attempted  to  "shoo" 
them  away  and  to  stop  their  cackling;  a  month  later  a  swelling 
of  his  feet  and  legs  was  observed,  and  at  that  time  he  was  fre- 
quently found  on  his  hands  and  knees  on  the  floor  looking  under 
the  bed  for  the  dogs  which  he  believed  were  there  and  upon 
which  he  stepped;  at  this  time  he  admitted  having  auditory 
hallucinations  (of  voices)  ;  and  also  visual  hallucinations  (of 
ghosts  and  people)  when  he  closed  his  eyes;  he  assumed  catatonic 
attitudes;  he  exhibited  numerous  mannerisms,  and  at  times  had 
impulsive  outbreaks;  at  the  same  time  he  was  negativistic ;  he 
was  found  to  have  pulmonary  tuberculosis,  and  death  was  due 
to  this. 

In  addition,  the  autopsy  showed:  intestinal  tuberculosis  and 
some  shrinkage  of  the  cerebral  convolutions  anteriorly. 

Case  8,  white  male,  had  exhibited  mental  disturbance  for  more 
than  6  months  previous  to  his  admission;  he  was  aged  30,  and 
lived  2  years  and  8  months  in  the  Hospital. 

His  /family  history  was  negative;  the  patient  had  attempted 
a  criminal  assault  and  was  convicted  for  this;  in  the  last  few 
months  of  his  term  in  prison  he  developed  a  disorderly  tempera- 
ment ;  he  laughed  when  questions  were  asked ;  he  refused  to  wear 


SYMPTOMATOLOGICAL  DIFFERENCES  IN  THE  INSANE      17 

clothing  and  to  obey  orders,  and  occasionally  to  eat.  The  physi- 
cal and  neurological  examinations  revealed  nothing  of  importance 
except  a  distended,  tympanitic  abdomen  and  stumbling  over  test 
speech  phrases.  On  admission  he  conversed  with  a  physician 
pleasantly,  but  appeared  to  be  suspicious;  he  had  notable  man- 
nerisms of  gritting  his  teeth  and  drumming  on  the  table  with 
his  fingers ;  he  was  neat  and  tidy ;  he  believed  his  family  had  been 
sent  to  the  same  prison  as  he;  at  times  he  became  irritable  and 
was  pugnacious,  especially  when  another  patient  walked  in  front 
of  him,  and  because  of  this  he  had  several  altercations  with  them; 
he  expressed  the  belief  that  he  was  some  kind  of  a  Mason,  al- 
though he  had  not  been  initiated  into  or  by  a  regular  lodge,  but 
had  been  "admitted  through  another  personality" ;  his  replies  in- 
dicated that  he  believed  his  personality  changed  from  day  to 
day,  although  he  said  he  could  not  reveal  the  secret  of  this  be- 
cause it  was  Masonic  and  military;  in  general  he  was  well 
oriented  and  showed  fair  memory  and  considerable  general 
knowledge,  but  he  had  no  insight  into  his  condition;  he  refused 
to  answer  many  questions  on  the  ground  that  they  were  "too 
easy";  at  various  times  he  said  that  the  Government  owed  him 
money,  that  he  was  a  rich  man,  and  that  he  used  to  travel 
about  a  great  deal ;  he  continued  to  be  reticent  about  his  previous 
life  and  his  ideas,  but  was  cross  and  disagreeable;  he  refused 
medicine  and  food,  and  many  times  fought  with  the  attendants 
and  with  the  other  patients ;  hallucinations  of  any  kind  were  not 
recorded  as  having  been  evident;  he  frequently  complained  of 
abdominal  pain  and  suffered  from  distention  of  abdomen;  an 
operation  was  planned,  but  not  performed  on  account  of  the  sud- 
den death  of  the  patient  from  intestinal  obstruction  and  gangrene. 
In  addition  to  the  diseased  conditions  which  caused  his  death, 
the  autopsy  revealed  fatty  changes  in  the  liver  and  slight  shrink- 
age of  the  anterior  portions  of  the  cerebrum,  with  no  other  gross 
cerebral  lesions. 

Case  9,  white  male,  admitted  at  the  age  of  34  and  lived  nearly 
20  years  in  the  Hospital. 

The  mental  diagnosis  which  was  made  was  "probably  cerebral 
syphilis,"  but  later  this  was  changed  to  dementia  precox,  prob- 


i8  SHEPHERD  IVORY  FRANZ 

ably  paranoid  form.  The  certificate  on  entrance  stated  that  the 
patient  had  visual  hallucinations  and  delusions  of  persecution  but 
no  maniacal  explosions,  although  he  talked  incessantly  about  the 
loss  of  some  musical  instruments;  he  also  had  hallucinations, 
which  were  nocturnal  in  character,  of  seeing  women  coming  to 
the  beds  of  patients  who  were  in  the  same  ward  with  him.  He 
admitted  having  had  a  soft  chancre  at  eighteen,  and  gonorrhea  at 
nineteen ;  the  physical  examination  was  negative  except  for  irreg- 
ularities and  inequality  of  the  pupils;  there  was  no  disorder  of 
voluntary  movement,  or  of  the  reflexes  or  of  sensation ;  there  were 
hypertrophy  of  the  heart  and  lesions  of  the  valves.  He  smiled  con- 
stantly, his  expression  was  dreamy  but  fairly  intelligent;  no 
peculiar  actions  were  noted;  he  reported  that  soon  after  his  ar- 
rival at  the  Hospital  he  was  given  some  black  medicine  which 
injured  his  health,  making  him  nervous  and  giving  him  pains  in 
the  legs  like  needles,  and  causing  him  to  spit,  which  made  his 
tongue  sore;  subsequently,  no  delusions  were  elicited  except  a 
brief  mention  of  this  supposed  poisoning  episode.  Thirteen  years 
after  his  entrance,  it  was  noted  that  he  was  not  depressed;  he 
talked  and  associated  with  the  other  patients;  he  played  games 
of  different  kinds  when  on  the  ward,  and  also  played  a  musical 
instrument;  he  sat  quietly;  he  was  not  overly  religious;  he  did 
not  assume  peculiar  attitudes ;  he  was  tidy  in  habits ;  he  did  not 
lack  in  initiative;  his  memory  for  recent  and  old  events  was 
good ;  he  was  not  agitated  as  a  result  of  any  of  his  delusions ;  he 
did  not  become  excited,  abusive,  profane,  destructive  or  untidy; 
he  slept  well;  his  conversation  was  coherent;  he  had  parole  of  the 
grounds;  he  was  well  oriented;  he  appeared  to  be  content;  he 
never  complained,  and  he  played  in  the  Hospital  band.  On 
several  occasions  later  he  became  restless  and  very  much  con- 
fused, being  unable  to  comprehend  what  was  said  to  him  and 
seeming  to  be  much  disturbed ;  one  of  these  attacks  immediately 
preceded  his  death.  During  the  later  years  of  his  life  he  showed 
marked  loss  of  intelligence  and  very  little  judgment;  it  was  diffi- 
cult to  get  his  attention,  and  when  his  attention  was  obtained, 
it  was  almost  impossible  to  hold  it ;  he  showed  much  retardation  ; 
he  spoke  indistinctly,  and  at  times  his  teeth  were  kept  closed  so 


SYMPTOMATOLOGICAL  DIFFERENCES  IN  THE  INSANE      ig 

that  it  was  difficult  to  understand  what  he  said;  he  also  seemed 
unable  to  comprehend  some  of  the  simplest  questions,  and  when 
asked  to  repeat  one  which  had  been  asked,  he  was  unable  to  do 
so;  he  remained  orderly,  but  took  no  interest  in  his  surroundings; 
he  was  neat  and  tidy;  he  was  also  fairly  well  oriented  in  all 
spheres :  he  had  fair  memory  of  his  personal  history,  but  of  other 
events  his  memory  was  very  poor. 

Death  was  due  to  cardiac  valvular  disease.  The  autopsy 
showed  that  he  had:  hypertrophied  heart;  marked  aortic  ather- 
oma ;  mitral  aortic  valves  contracted ;  pulmonary  tubercular  scars 
and  pulmonary  hypostasis;  nutmeg  liver;  some  adhesions  in  the 
region  of  the  appendix;  brain  shrinkage  over  the  anterior  two- 
thirds  of  the  convolutions,  but  no  other  cerebral  lesions;  and  a 
slight  sclerosis  of  the  large  vessels  at  the  base. 

DEMENTIA  PRECOX,  DISCUSSION 

Although  these  nine  cases  have  somewhat  different  clinical 
symptoms,  the  symptomatology  of  all  is  sufficiently  alike  to  war- 
rant the  clinical  diagnosis  of  dementia  precox.  The  form  of 
the  disease,  it  should  be  remarked,  is  not  the  same  in  all  cases, 
five  being  judged  to  be  catatonic  (3,  4,  5,  6,  and  7),  and  two  to 
be  paranoid  (i  and  8).  Cases  2  and  9  were  committed  to  the 
Hospital  many  years  ago.  The  case  histories  of  these  patients 
contained  no,  or  very  meagre,  information  regarding  their  mental 
conditions  previous  to  commitment,  and  only  outlines  of  their 
behavior  during  their  early  hospital  residence.  We  are  not  en- 
tirely justified  in  making  a  definite  diagnosis  of  the  form  of 
precox  from  the  information  obtained  in  their  later  years,  but 
the  general  diagnosis  of  precox  is,  however,  justified  by  the  ac- 
counts which  have  been  kept,  and  the  symptoms  recorded  in  the 
records  during  their  later  years  might  be  interpreted  to  indicate 
that  case  2  was  a  paranoid  case,  and  case  9  an  hebephrenia. 

Looking  at  these  cases  from  another  point  of  view,  it  will  be 
noted  that  the  anatomical  lesions  do  not  correspond  with  the 
clinical  forms.  Of  the  three  cases  in  which  the  brain  was  noted 
to  have  shown  frontal  atrophy  one  was  diagnosed  as  paranoid,  a 
second  as  an  hebephrenic  and  the  third  was  a  "possible  paranoid" 


20  SHEPHERD  IVORY  FRANZ 

case.  Of  the  five  cases  with  anterior  atrophy  the  mental  diagno- 
sis of  catatonia  was  made  in  four,  while  the  fifth  was  a  paranoid 
case.  The  ninth  case,  with  atrophy  covering  the  anterior  two- 
thirds  of  the  cerebral  convolutions  was  the  case  mentioned  above 
as  possibly  hebephrenic.  The  only  evidence  of  a  possible  corre- 
lation between  the  forms  of  precox  and  the  character  of  the 
cerebral  atrophy  is  the  fact  that  most  of  the  cases  of  lesions  of 
the  anterior  convolutions  are  catatonic.  This  might  be  taken  to 
indicate  that  motor  disturbances  in  catatonia  may  be  associated 
with  pathological  changes  in  the  anterior  portions  of  the  cere- 
brum, but  case  8,  whose  brain  also  exhibited  similar  lesions  did 
not  exhibit  these  behavior  disorders.  It  should  also  be  remem- 
bered that  case  9,  in  whom  the  atrophy  covered  slightly  more  of 
the  cortex  than  in  the  catatonics  did  not  exhibit  motor  disturb- 
ances of  the  nature  of  catatonia.  These  two  cases  (8  and  9)  are 
sufficiently  definite  to  prevent  a  generalization  regarding  the  re- 
lations of  such  anterior  lesions  with  motor  disturbances  of  the 
nature  of  catatonia  or  negativism. 

During  their  Hospital  residence  all  of  these  patients  were 
noted  to  show  gradual  mental  deterioration,  although  the  amount 
of  this  decadence  differed  in  the  individual  cases.  The  histories 
show  that  cases  2,  3,  4,  6,  and  9  showed  towards  the  end  less 
evidence  of  being  thinking  beings  than  the  other  four  cases 
showed.  Excluding  case  4,  whose  age  at  the  time  of  death  was 
seventy-two,  and  whose  lack  of  mentality  might  have  been  due  to 
the  natural  decadence  associated  with  old  age,  the  other  four  cases 
are  not  chronologically  aged,  and,  in  fact,  of  the  other  cases 
there  were  two  (i  and  7)  whose  ages  were  respectively  greater 
than  those  of  cases  3,  6,  and  9,  and  case  3.  While  the  degree  of 
atrophy  in  these  five  cases  is  not  specified,  even  roughly,  there  is 
nothing  to  indicate  any  definite  relation  between  the  greater  men- 
tal deterioration  and  the  degree  of  atrophy.  It  is  true  that  the 
brains  of  cases  i,  5,  and  8  are  described  as  showing  only  "slight" 
atrophy,  but  case  7,  in  whom  extreme  mental  changes  were  not 
found  was  also  described  as  showing  "some"  atrophy,  which  term 
may  be  interpreted,  as  I  interpret  it  here,  to  indicate  only  a 
medium  degree,  rather  than  a  slight  degree.  No  mathematical 


SYMPTOMATOLOGICAL  DIFFERENCES  IN  THE  INSANE      21 

estimation  has  been  made  in  the  individual  cases  regarding  either 
atrophy  or  mental  deterioration,  and  the  data  at  hand  do  not 
permit  the  correlation  of  these  two  conditions  at  the  present  time. 

It  is,  however,  of  interest  and  importance  to  note  that  the 
degree  of  dementia  in  those  cases  in  which  the  frontal  lobes  were 
atrophied  did  not  differ  to  any  appreciable  extent  from  those 
cases  in  which  the  atrophic  regions  were  larger.  Thus  case  2 
has  been  described  as  exhibiting  no  knowledge  of  things  occur- 
ring about  him,  and  case  3  to  have  "very  little  mental  capacity," 
although  the  latter  patient  at  the  time  of  his  death  was  only  forty- 
two  years  old.  In  addition,  it  has  been  noted  that  case  i  had  a 
poor  memory. 

Although  the  degree  of  dementia,  or  mental  abnormality,  does 
not  appear  to  be  directly  correlated  either  with  the  extent  or  with 
the  degree  of  the  atrophy  in  the  cases  which  we  have  studied,  we 
may  seek  for  correlations  in  the  individual  mental  symptoms. 
Differing  in  the  individual  cases,  and  giving,  as  they  do,  special 
characteristics  to  the  disease,  the  mental  symptoms  can  not  be 
dealt  with  in  great  detail.  Nor  can  the  individual  elements  of 
the  mental  processes  be  considered,  for  in  the  clinical  histories 
the  complex  mental  states  or  processes  have  not  been  analyzed 
into  their  elements.  This  is,  however,  not  different  from  the 
methodological  condition  found  in  most  clinical  work,  e.g.,  in 
neurology,  and  the  attempts  at  correlation  of  the  complex  proc- 
esses with  cerebral  lesions  may  therefore  well  be  attempted  re- 
gardless of  the  lack  of  analysis  of  these  complexes. 

Underlying  all  diagnostic  methods  in  psychiatry  are  the  as- 
sumptions that  mental  states  are  mirrored  by  acts,  that  acts 
change  in  accordance  with  the  mental  states,  and  that  changes  in 
mentality,  which  are  supposed  to  be  produced  by  or  correlated 
with  functional  or  anatomical  cerebral  lesions,  are  evidenced  by 
alterations  in  general  behavior.  In  their  general  form  these  as- 
sumptions may  be  satisfactory,  but  when  they  are  made  more 
specific  they  become  open  to  criticism.  In  the  cases  which  have 
been  described  above  this  latter  appears  to  be  true.  It  will  be 
noticed  that  four  of  these  patients  have  been  described  as  tidy 
in  habits,  while  four  others  have  been  described  as  untidy  or 


22  SHEPHERD  IVORY  FRANZ 

filthy,  while  the  ninth  has  not  been  specifically  described  but  ap- 
pears to  have  been  tidy.  Two  of  the  untidy  patients  were  those 
in  which  only  the  frontal  regions  were  atrophied,  and  two  in 
which  the  anterior  lobes  were  shrunken.  Case  9,  in  whom  the 
atrophy  extended  farther  backwards  than  in  the  other  eight 
cases,  was,  however,  noted  to  be  tidy.  In  this  respect  there  is 
correlation  neither  with  the  extent  nor  with  the  degree  of  the 
atrophy. 

Although  the  frontal  lobes  are  believed  to  have  more  direct 
relations  with  motor  processes,  the  data  at  hand  regarding  the 
motor  manifestations,  other  than  the  catatonic  attitudes,  etc., 
in  these  patients  do  not  appear  to  be  correlated  with  the  lesions. 
A  comparison  of  the  movement  differences  in  the  patients  under 
consideration  shows  that  the  variations  are  not  variations  cor- 
responding with  the  regional  atrophies.  Certain  of  the  patients 
varied  from  a  quiet  to  a  restless  state;  others  were  almost  con- 
tinually restless  or  noisy;  and  two  were  noted  to  be  uniformly 
slow,  unresponsive,  and,  during  part  of  the  time,  motionless. 
Some  may  be  inclined  to  interpret  the  general  motor  manifesta- 
tions to  be  evidence  of  a  supposed  inhibition  function  of  the 
frontal  regions  of  the  brain,  in  some  of  the  cases  there  being  a 
greater  amount  of  inhibition,  and  in  others  a  lesser  amount  of 
inhibition  than  in  normal  individuals.  The  fact  that  the  vari- 
ations were  not  always  in  the  same  direction  in  the  different 
patients,  would,  however,  be  a  matter  needing  special  explanation 
in  addition  to  the  general  hypothesis.  We  should,  on  the  basis 
of  our  present  knowledge  of  the  motor  functions  of  the  cortex, 
be  able  to  explain  or  to  correlate  these  irregular  weekly  or 
monthly  variations  in  activity  in  the  same  patient  with  exaccerba- 
tions  in  irritation  or  degeneration  of  the  ganglion  cells,  but  the 
pathological  facts  which  would  warrant  such  explanations  or 
correlations  are  lacking.  One  element,  which  appears  to  the 
writer  to  be  important  in  this  connection  is  that  the  motor  phe- 
norfiena  in  the  cases  under  consideration  did  not  differ  very 
greatly.  If  these  motor  disorders  were  due  principally  to  the 
pathological  conditions  of  the  cells  in  the  atrophic  areas,  we 
should  expect  to  find  greater  motor  disturbances  associated  with 


those  atrophies  which  were  of  the  greatest  extent,  or  at  least  we 
should  expect  that  the  motor  activities  in  those  patients  in  whom 
the  precentral,  or  physiological  motor,  area  was  involved  should 
be  more  greatly  changed  than  in  those  in  which  only  the  frontal 
lobes  were  atrophied.  Such,  however,  is  not  the  case.  There  is, 
as  has  been  written  above,  no  apparent  difference  between  the 
motor  derangements  in  those  patients  with  frontal  lesions  and 
those  with  lesions  which  also  involved  the  precentral  areas,  nor 
even  between  the  motor  phenomena  in  the  case  with  frontal 
lesions  and  those  in  the  one  case  in  which  the  atrophy  also  in- 
volved the  precentral  areas  and  parts  of  the  parietals  in  addition 
to  the  frontals. 

Certain  clinico-neurological  facts  have  been  interpreted  to 
mean  that  the  activities  of  the  frontal  lobes  are  especially  asso- 
ciated with  emotional  states  or  emotional  expression,  and  were 
this  true,  different  degrees  of  frontal  lesions  might  be  expected 
to  result  in  variations  in  emotional  tone  or  in  the  character  of 
the  affective  conditions.  In  these  nine  cases,  however,  the  emo- 
tional conditions  were  somewhat  similar,. in  that  it  was  variable 
in  all.  It  varied  from  depression  to  indifference,  and  occasion- 
ally to  a  high  degree  of  happiness.  Many  of  the  affective  states 
in  these  patients  depended  upon,  or  resulted  from,  or,  to  speak 
accurately,  accompanied,  and  corresponded  with,  delusions,  and 
it  is  not  possible  to  separate  the  affective  elements  from  these 
other  mental  states.  Neither  the  degree  nor  the  extent  of  the 
cerebral  changes  appears  to  be  correlated  with  the  intensity, 
character  or  variability  of  the  affective  states,  for  the  fear  and 
suspiciousness  of  cases  I  and  5,  in  which  only  slight  atrophies 
respectively  of  the  frontal  and  anterior  convolutions  were  noted, 
did  not  differ  from  similar  states  in  patients  3  and  7,  whose 
brains  showed  corresponding  areal  distributions,  but  with  slightly 
increased  degrees,  of  atrophy.  An  examination  of  the  case  his- 
tories also  shows  that  the  fluctuations  from  one  affective  state 
to  another  is  not  associated  with  particular  degrees  or  extents  of 
the  lesions. 

Notable  variations  both  in  the  presence  and  in  the  characters  of 
hallucinations  were  also  recorded.  Of  the  nine  patients,  four 


24  SHEPHERD  IVORY  FRANZ 

exhibited  plain  evidence  of  auditory  hallucinations  (cases  I,  2, 
5,  and  7).  Two  others  (cases  4  and  6)  appear  to  have  had  audi- 
tory hallucinations,  although  this  is  not  as  certain  as  in  the  other 
four  cases.  The  presence  of  auditory  hallucinations  in  case  4 
has  been  inferred  from  his  talking  to  himself,  and  similarly  in 
case  6  because  he  mumbled  to  himself  and  because  he  had  been 
noted  to  turn  his  head  towards  a  special  part  of  the  room  and 
appeared  to  listen  when  no  one  was  present  there.  Definite  evi- 
dence of  visual  hallucinations  was  found  in  only  two  of  the  cases ; 
viz.,  patients  7  and  9,  and  in  one  of  these  it  has  been  noted  they 
were  more  frequently  present  at  night.  Patients  I  and  7  also 
exhibited  actions  which  were  interpreted  to  mean  that  they  had 
tactual  hallucinations  or  paresthesias.  Patient  i,  it  will  be  re- 
membered, reported  that  she  could  give  out  electrical  power,  and 
that  electric  currents  had  been  turned  upon  her.  Patient  7  com- 
plained of  chickens  and  dogs  which  he  thought  were  in  his  room 
and  which  he  was  compelled  to  step  upon  when  he  got  out  of  his 
bed.  Whether  or  not  the  latter  case  is  a  mixed  hallucination, 
e.g.,  tactual  and  visual,  was  not  determined.  The  actions  of  the 
patient  indicated  that  he  did  not  see  the  animals,  for  he  searched 
for  them  under  his  bed.  Case  3  was  reported  to  have  had  hal- 
lucinations previous  to  his  entrance  to  the  hospital,  but  during 
his  Hospital  residence  these  were  not  discovered.  In  the  case 
history  of  patient  8,  in  whose  brain  the  atrophy  extended  over 
the  anterior  portions,  no  hallucinations  were  recorded  at  any 
time. 

With  the  exception  of  cases  3  and  4,  these  patients  were  noted 
to  have  delusions.  It  may  not  be  definitely  stated  that  patient  3 
did  not  have  a  delusion  of  any  kind,  for  there  is  internal  evi- 
dence in  his  case  history  that  mental  abnormalities  of  this  kind 
may  have  been  present.  It  is  stated,  for  example,  that  in  the 
year  preceding  his  commitment  to  the  Hospital  he  was  restless 
and  melancholy  and  exhibited  extreme  anxiety.  The  reasons  for 
these  affective  states  are  not  mentioned,  but  it  appears  doubtful 
that  they  were  independent  of  ideas  of  impending  harm,  or  of 
persecution  or  the  like.  None  of  his  actions  during  his  Hospital 
residence  was  indicative  of  the  presence  of  a  delusion,  although 


during  those  years  he  was  observed  to  fluctuate  between  excite- 
ment and  mutism.  Case  4  was  also  judged  to  be  free  from 
delusions  during  the  period  of  his  Hospital  residence,  although 
the  general  facts  regarding  his  mental  condition  for  21  years 
previous  to  his  admission  are  not  sufficient  to  warrant  the  state- 
ment that  delusions  had  not  been  present  during  that  period  of 
time.  The  considerable  degree  of  dementia  which  was  present 
during  his  Hospital  residence  may  have  made  the  expression  of 
delusions  difficult,  but  his  reactions  to  the  auditory  hallucinations 
from  which  he  suffered  would  not  lead  to  this  conclusion.  The 
characters  of  the  delusions  of  the  other  seven  cases  ranged  over 
the  fields  of  somatopsychic,  autopsychic,  and  allopsychic.  De- 
lusions of  persecution  were  the  more  frequent,  but  these  alter- 
nated with  delusions  of  grandeur  in  some  cases.  None  of  the 
somatopsychic  delusions  was  definitely  associated  with  corre- 
sponding pathological  physical  conditions,  with  the  possible  ex- 
ception of  those  of  case  7.  This  patient  complained  that  chickens 
and  dogs  were  on  the  floor  of  his  room,  and  that  he  had  to  step 
upon  them  when  he  rose  from  his  bed.  The  physical  condition 
associated  with  this  delusion  was  a  swelling  of  the  feet,  and  as 
has  been  suggested  above,  the  hallucinations  and  the  delusion 
may  have  resulted  from  the  stretching  of  the  skin.  The  as- 
sociation of  the  different  characters  of  delusions  with  the  cere- 
bral atrophies  in  these  cases  is  neither  constant  nor  definite,  cases 
3  and  4  belonging  respectively  to  the  frontal  and  anterior  atrophy 
groups,  so  that  we  are  forced  to  the  conclusion  that  the  lesions 
can  not  be  constantly  associated  with  delusion  formation.  These 
results  are  of  interest  in  connection  with  Southard's  studies  of 
delusions  and  especially  with  his  conclusion  that  the  presence  of 
delusions  is  "to  be  correlated  more  with  lesions  of  the  anterior 
association  center."4 

Regarding  the  conditions  of  memory  in  these  patients  there  is 
little  information.  Six  of  them  were  unable  to  recount  recent 
events  or  events  in  their  own  lives,  or  were  unable  to  repeat 
simple  questions  which  had  previously  been  asked.  The  other 

4  Southard,  E.  E.  The  (Mind  Twist  and  Brain  Spot  Hypotheses  in  Psycho- 
pathology  and  Neuropathology.  Psych.  Bull.,  ,1914,  11,  117-130.  See  espe- 
cially p.  123,  and  other  references  there  given. 


26  SHEPHERD  IVORY  FRANZ 

three  cases  exhibited  a  fair  degree  of  memory.  Whether  these 
apparent  memory  defects  were  due  to  lack  of  attention  or  to 
inability  to  retain  the  impressions  which  were  received  can  not 
be  decided.  All  the  patients  with  simple  frontal  lesions,  irre- 
spective of  the  degree  of  atrophy,  had  poor  memory,  whereas 
patients  4,  7,  and  8  with  anterior  atrophies  exhibited  fair  memory. 

In  contrast  to  the  memory  defects  of  these  patients  the  degree 
of  orientation  is  to  be  noted.  Although  orientation  depends,  at 
least  to  a  certain  extent,  upon  retention,  it  is  not  necessarily  as- 
sociated with  general  memory  ability.  This  is  shown  in  case  9 
where  memory  appeared  to  be  very  defective,  although  he  was 
fairly  well  oriented  in  all  spheres,  and  in  case  5  who,  although, 
disoriented  for  time,  was  oriented  for  place  and  persons.  On 
the  other  hand  patient  4,  who  exhibited  a  fair  degree  of  memory, 
had  no  grasp  of  his  surroundings.  The  relation  of  orientation 
ability  to  the  lesions  in  the  nine  cases  is  not  definite,  case  I  being 
opposed  in  this  particular  to  cases  2  and  3,  and  cases  5,  7,  and 
8,  showing  good  orientation  while  the  corresponding  cases,  4  and 
6,  were  in  general  disoriented.  Case  9,  in  which  the  atrophy  ex- 
tended beyond  the  anterior  region,  was  also  noted  to  be  fairly 
well  oriented. 

The  ability  of  the  patients  to  attend  to  stimuli  is  not  mentioned 
in  all  of  the  case  histories,  but  reading  between  the  lines,  it  is 
evident  that  this  was  poor.  It  is  possible  that  some  of  the  ap- 
parent memory  defects  were  due  to  lack  of  attention,  and  that 
certain  of  the  other  mental  disturbances  were  also  caused  by  the 
failure  to  attend  to  stimuli.  It  is  also  possible  that  the  degree  of 
dementia  is  correlated  to  a  certain  extent  with  the  failure  to 
attend. 

When  the  facts  of  atrophy  and  the  facts  of  mental  abnormali- 
ties in  these  nine  patients  are  taken  together,  it  is  seen  that  there 
are  decided  differences  in  the  character  of  the  psychic  symptoms 
associated  with  similar  cerebral  lesions.  It  is  also  apparent  that 
regardless,  of  the  extent  of  the  atrophies  similar  mental  symp- 
toms may  be  evidenced.  The  conclusion  that  follows,  therefore, 
is  that  similar  cerebral  lesions  in  cases  of  dementia  precox  do 
not  always  result  in  similar  forms  of  the  disease,  nor  in  similar 
symptoms  in  all  individuals,  nor  in  the  same  degree  of  dementia.. 


GENERAL  PARALYSIS  OF  THE  INSANE,   CLINICAL 

HISTORIES 

Case  10,  colored  male,  was  admitted  to  the  Hospital  at  the  age 
of  30,  and  lived  over  4^  years. 

There  is  nothing  of  importance  in  the  patient's  family  history, 
and  the  only  facts  of  interest  in  his  personal  history  is  that  he 
admitted  gonorrhea,  denied  having  had  syphilis,  and  admitted 
that  he  had  used  alcoholic  liquors  since  he  was  a  boy,  but  said 
he  had  never  been  drunk.     The  physical  •  examination  revealed 
nothing  of  interest,  except  that  there  was  a  complete  positive 
Wassermann  of  the  blood  serum  and  a  trace  of  the  reaction  in 
the  cerebrospinal  fluid.    He  had  diminished  knee  jerks,  sluggish 
reactions  and  inequalities  of  the  pupils,  well-marked  tremor  of 
the  tongue,  lips  and  facial  muscles ;  speech  was  somewhat  ataxic, 
and  there  was  difficulty  in  repeating  test  phrases.    The  onset  of 
the  mental  disturbance  appeared  to  be  a  sudden  one;  he  had 
been  working  as  a  waiter  two  days  before  his  arrest;  he  said 
he  had  found  a  check  which  called  for  $30,000,000,  on  the  back 
of  which  there  was  a  notice  to  return  it  to  the  bank  and  receive 
$1,000,000  for  it;  the  bank  was  closed  but  he  showed  it  to  a 
number  of  people  and  tried  to  get  $300  for  it;  one  man  gave 
him  $500  for  it,  but  a  policeman  arrested  the  patient,  took  him 
to  a  hospital  and  later  he  was  brought  to  this  institution.    How 
much  of  the  above  incident  was  based  upon  a  minimum  of  fact 
was  not  determinable.     On  entrance  his  expression  was  one  of 
exaltation;  he  was  quiet  and  cheerful;  his  habits  were  tidy;  he 
gave  no  trouble  except  slightly  by  constantly  requesting  that  he 
be  permitted  to  be  allowed  to  go  out  so  that  he  could  get  his 
money;  he  was  well  oriented  for  time,  place,  and  persons;  his 
attention  could  easily  be  attracted  and  held;  he  comprehended 
what  was  said  to  him,  and  answered  coherently  and  relevantly, 
except  when  his  delusional  ideas  were  touched  upon,  whereupon 
he  became  rambling  and  disconnected  in  conversation;  his  mem- 


28  SHEPHERD  IVORY  FRANZ 

ory  for  recent  and  remote  events  was  good  for  one  of  his  grade 
of  intelligence ;  his  emotional  tone  was  one  of  exaltation,  he  was 
as  "happy  as  a  lark";  he  was  sociable  with  the  other  patients 
and  worked  on  the  ward;  no  hallucinations  could  be  elicited. 
Later,  he  had  a  period  of  confusion  for  a  few  hours,  in  which 
he  took  the  dishes  from  the  table  and  placed  them  outside  of 
the  window  (reporting  that  he  thought  thereby  he  would  get 
some  tobacco),  and  attacked  one  of  the  nurses  and  demanded 
his  keys;  his  delusions  of  wealth  had  not  changed  materially; 
subsequently  he  had  convulsions,  was  then  confined  to  bed,  and 
developed  contractures.  At  that  time  he  spoke  rarely  and  then 
only  a  few  phrases,  he  took  no  cognizance  of  what  went  on 
and  led  a  nearly  vegetable  existence  till  his  death  'from  exhaus- 
tion of  paresis.  The  autopsy  showed  that  the  cerebral  convolu- 
tions over  the  frontal  regions  were  shrunken. 

Case  n,  colored  male,  had  been  known  to  have 
mental  abnormalities  ,for  at  least  7  months  prior  to  his  ad- 
mission at  the  age  of  37,  and  he  lived  for  nearly  2  years  in  the 
Hospital. 

His  family  and  personal  history  were  not  obtained  in  any 
detail,  for  he  could  not  give  much  information  and  what  he  gave 
appeared  to  be  rather  unreliable.  He  denied  a  syphilitic  history, 
but  was  found  to  have  a  scar  on  his  penis  due  to  an  old  sore ;  the 
Wassermann  reaction  with  the  blood  serum  was  complete  posi- 
tive, with  the  cerebrospinal  fluid  a  trace,  the  number  of  cells  in 
the  cerebrospinal  fluid  was  170  per  cu.  mm.  The  neurological 
examination  showed  marked  tremors  of  the  tongue  and  fingers, 
and  somewhat  generally  throughout  the  body;  station  and  gait 
were  tremulous  and  impaired,  he  was  unable  to  stand  on  one  leg; 
coordination  was  impaired;  there  was  a  marked  speech  defect; 
some  of  the  reflexes  were  diminished,  others  were  absent;  the 
pupils  were  unequal  and  reacted  only  slightly  to  stimuli.  He 
appeared  to  be  contented  and  did  not  seem  to  worry ;  he  sat  in  a 
listless,  though  cheerful,  mood,  and  paid  little  attention  to  his 
surroundings ;  he  did  not  appear  to  be  oriented ;  he  obeyed  simple 


29 

commands,  but  when  questioned  he  did  not  answer  at  times; 
he  was  tidy  in  his  dress  and  in  his  habits  and  did  not  conflict  with 
his  surroundings;  he  believed  he  owned  a  race  horse  for  which 
he  had  paid  $3,000.  He  had  a  brief  attack  of  unconsciousness 
followed  by  a  .period  of  confusion  in  which  he  talked  to  himself 
and  made  signs  with  his  hands.  Physically  and  mentally  he 
declined,  until  he  was  unable  to  do  anything  for  himself;  he 
replied  to  no  questions,  and  was  apparently  oblivious  of  his 
surroundings.  Death  was  due  to  exhaustion  of  paresis.  The 
autopsy  also  revealed  pulmonary  tuberculosis,  pericarditis, 
atrophy  of  the  heart  valves,  fatty  degeneration  of  the  liver, 
chronic  cystitis,  and  general  shrinkage  of  the  frontal  lobes.  The 
microscopical  examination  showed  that  the  frontal  and  central 
convolutions  exhibited  the  characteristic  pictures  of  paresis,  but 
that  the  parietal  lobes  did  not  show  any  marked  changes. 

Case  12,  white  male,  had  been  suffering  from  mental  disease 
for  at  least  a  year  previous  to  his  admission  at  the  age  of  51 ; 
he  lived  2  years  and  8  months  after  his  admission. 

The  only  fact  of  interest  in  the  family  history  was  that  his 
mother  had  died  of  pulmonary  tuberculosis  when  the  patient  was 
eight  months  old.  He  received  a  high  school  education,  and  after 
serving  for  eight  years  in  the  army,  was  a  clerk.  He  had  con- 
tracted syphilis  thirty  years  previous  to  his  admission  and  had 
gonorrhea  at  a  later  period,  but  subsequently  married  and 
begot  three  healthy  children;  he  used  tobacco  to  excess.  About 
a  year  previous  to  his  admission  he  became  mentally  fatigued 
very  easily,  his  memory  was  impaired ;  he  could  not  comprehend 
as  well  as  he  formerly  could;  his  speech  became  indistinct;  his 
writing  was  careless,  showing  many  omissions  and  mistakes  in 
spelling ;  his  gait  was  ataxic.  At  the  same  time  he  became  morose, 
sullen  and  irritable;  he  worried  over  the  state  of  his  health; 
he  made  remarks  about  killing  the  members  of  his  family  and 
himself ;  he  also  bought  immense  quantities  of  perfumed  soap  and 
bathed  constantly.  The  neurological  examination  showed 
coarse  tremors  of  the  facial  muscles  and  twitchings  of  the  thigh 
muscles;  Romberg  sign  was  present;  gait  was  unsteady;  the 


30  SHEPHERD  IVORY  FRANZ 

knee  jerks  were  not  elicited;  the  Wassermann  reaction  with  the 
cerebrospinal  fluid  was  complete  positive ;  the  number  of  cells  per 
cu.  mm.  was  20.  On  entrance  to  the  Hospital  he  appeared  to 
be  cheerful,  contented  and  appreciative;  but  was  inclined  to 
be  fault-finding  with  his  surroundings  and  fretful  that  he  was 
not  permitted  to  have  all  his  clothes  and  his  special  toilet  articles 
in  his  room ;  he  kept  busy  washing  his  hands  and  face  and  comb- 
ing his  hair ;  he  appeared  to  be  bewildered  at  times,  and  expressed 
the  belief  that  he  had  offended  some  one  and  he  worried  about 
this;  in  general,  he  showed  no  interest  in  what  went  on  about 
him;  his  memory  for  recent  and  remote  events  was  poor;  no 
evidence  of  the  existence  of  hallucinations  or  delusions  was  dis- 
covered; his  habits  were  tidy;  he  was  usually  quiet,  but  at 
times  was  nervous.  Later,  fantastic  and  ridiculous  delusions  of 
a  grandiose  type  were  evidenced,  e.g.,  he  was  to  take  a  marvelous 
train  ride  through  the  country,  he  had  invented  things,  etc.  At 
times  he  became  worried,  he  wanted  to  shoot  a  fellow  patient, 
whom  he  believed  to  be  an  enemy ;  he  imagined  people  were  going 
to  do  something  mean  to  him,  that  they  came  to  take  away  his 
trunk  in  which  he  had  his  money,  he  also  spoke  of  being  tired  and 
explained  that  this  was  due  to  his  having  had  to  fight  negroes 
during  the  night;  he  became  untidy  in  habits.  The  depressive 
delusions  apparently  disappeared,  leaving  only  those  o.f  a  gran- 
diose type,  that  he  owned  the  Hospital,  had  immense  amount  of 
money,  etc.  He  became  much  demented,  he  was  bedridden,  con- 
tractures  of  the  left  arm  and  leg  developed,  there  were  no  con- 
vulsions; death  was  due  to  exhaustion  of  paresis.  The  brain 
showed  great  atrophy  in  the  frontal  lobes. 

Case  13,  colored  male,  admitted  to  the  Hospital  at  the  age  of 
41,  lived  for  3  years  and  2  months  subsequently.  The  duration 
of  mental  disease  at  the  time  of  admission  was  about  2  years. 

The  family  history  was  negative.  The  medical  certificate 
stated  that  the  patient  had  had  syphilis;  mentally  he  had  a  vio- 
lent temper  and  was  very  irritable ;  he  had  been  melancholy,  and 
had  exhibited  delusions  and  homicidal  tendencies.  Physical  ex- 


SYMPTOM  AT  OLOGICAL  DIFFERENCES  IN  THE  INSANE      31 

amination  showed  only  a  slight  heart  involvement,  Wassermann 
reactions  with  the  blood  serum  and  the  cerebrospinal  fluid  were 
positive,  there  were  65  cells  per  cu.  mm.  in  the  cerebrospinal 
fluid.  The  neurological  examination  showed  irregularities  in 
outline  of  the  pupils;  tremors  of  tongue,  lips,  eyelids,  and  ex- 
tended fingers ;  patellar  reflexes  were  very  much  diminished,  and 
there  was  a  Babinski  phenomenon  on  the  right;  his  station  was 
poor ;  there  was  ihyperextension  of  the  legs  at  the  knee ;  the  gait 
was  slightly  ataxic;  coordination  was  poor;  and  there  was  a 
marked  speech  defect.  His  facial  expression  was  placid;  he  was 
orderly  and  quiet,  he  had  little  or  nothing  to  say  voluntarily; 
he  was  indifferent  and  inclined  to  be  stupid;  his  attention  was 
easily  obtained  and  held;  he  comprehended  questions  and  an- 
swered coherently  and  correctly,  but  slowly ;  he  was  not  oriented 
for  place,  time,  or  people;  his  memory  was  very  defective;  his 
judgment  was  poor;  he  lacked  insight;  emotionally  he  was  neither 
exalted  nor  depressed,  but  rather  indifferent  and  apparently 
satisfied;  he  did  not  associate  with  the  other  patients  and  mani- 
fested no  interest  in  his  surroundings ;  no  delusions  or  hallucina- 
tions could  be  elicited,  nor  were  they  judged  to  be  present  because 
of  his  conduct.  Later  he  was  restless,  disturbed  and  at  times 
noisy ;  he  talked  to  imaginary  people,  but  recognized  no  one  who 
saw  him;  dementia  became  profound;  he  comprehended  little; 
his  conversation  was  rambling  and  incoherent;  occasionally  he 
expressed  some  words  indicating  that  he  had  grandiose  ideas,  that 
he  possessed  houses  and  diamonds ;  at  times  he  became  disturbed 
and  noisy;  and  often  was  restless,  destructive  and  filthy;  when  he 
walked,  his  gait  was  very  ataxic.  The  autopsy  examination  re- 
vealed shrinkage  of  the  cerebral  convolutions,  especially  in  the 
anterior  parts. 

Case  14,  white  male,  was  admitted  at  the  age  of  33  years,  and 
lived  8  months;  his  mental  disease  was  evident  for  more  than  2 
months  previous  to  his  admission. 

The  medical  certificate  stated  that  the  patient's  father  died 
of  tuberculosis,  but  this  was  the  only  interesting  point  in  the 
family  history.  A  comrade  reported  that  the  patient  had  acted 


32  SHEPHERD  IVORY  FRANZ 

peculiarly  for  several  months.  The  patient  denied  that  he  had 
used  alcoholic  liquors.  He  showed  tremors  of  the  lips,  tongue 
and  hand,  difficulty  in  speaking,  some  impairment  of  intellect, 
muscular  weakness,  diminished  knee  jerks,  and  Romberg  sign. 
The  Wassermann  reaction  with  the  blood  serum  was  completely 
positive,  the  cerebrospinal  fluid  contained  145  cells  per  cu.  mm. 
On  admission  the  patient  would  not  talk,  and  <his  lack  of  co- 
operation prevented  a  careful  examination  of  his  mental  condi- 
tion. He  had  a  very  stupid,  blank  expression,  stood  or  sat  in  a 
stiff,  unnatural  position  until  told  to  do  somethig;  several  times 
he  rose  from  his  chair  and  started  towards  the  window  as  if 
governed  by  some  idea,  but  at  command  he  readily  resumed  his 
seat;  jerkings  of  different  muscles  were  observed;  if  undisturbed, 
he  closed  his  eyes  as  if  going  to  sleep;  he  did  not  apparently 
know  how  to  hold  a  pencil  and  his  first  efforts  at  writing  were 
scribbles;  his  movements  at  unbuttoning  his  coat  were  inco- 
ordinate; there  was  no  evidence  of  apparent  purpose  fulness  in 
any  of  his  movements;  his  attention  was  obtained  and  held 
with  difficulty;  he  appeared  to  know  the  name  of  the  Hospital; 
on  account  of  his  non-cooperation  it  was  impossible  to  determine 
anything  about  the  character  of  his  memory,  or  the  presence 
of  hallucinations  or  delusions.  Later  it  was  noted  that  he  was 
tidy,  disoriented  for  time,  place  and  persons,  his  memory  was 
very  poor;  he  could  not  find  his  bed,  etc.;  he  did  not  answer 
questions  readily  and  seldom  talked.  He  was  confined  to  bed 
and  died  of  exhaustion.  The  autopsy  revealed  shrinkage  of  the 
anterior  cerebral  convolutions,  hypostatic  pneumonia,  intersti- 
tial nephritis  and  purulent  cystitis. 

Case  15,  white  male,  was  noted  for  one  month  previous  to 
his  admission  to  exhibit  signs  of  mental  abnormality;  his  age 
was  38,  and  he  lived  in  the  Hospital  for  I  year  and  6  months. 

The  family  history  obtained  from  the  patient  contained  noth- 
ing of  importance.  The  medical  certificate  accompanying  him 
stated  that  he  had  syphilis  six  years  previously,  but  this  was 
denied  by  the  patient;  the  Wassermann  reaction  with  the  blood 
serum  was  complete  positive,  also  with  the  cerebrospinal  fluid; 


SYMPTOMATOLOGICAL  DIFFERENCES  IN  THE  INSANE      33 

the  number  of  cells  in  the  cerebrospinal  fluid  was  16.5  per  cu.  mm. 
The  patient  had  tuberculosis  during  his  residence  in  the  Hospital. 
Previous  to  his  admission  his  ward  mates  in  another  institu- 
tion had  complained  of  his  irrational  conduct  and  conversation. 
He  had  a  feeling  of  well  being  and  of  importance,  with  delusions 
of  grandeur,  but  no  delusions  of  persecution  or  hallucinations; 
in  addition  to  the  tuberculosis  it  was  found  that  the  patient's 
pupils  were  unequal,  the  left  reacted  neither  directly  nor  con- 
sensually  to  light  stimuli;  the  patellar  reflexes  could  not  be 
elicited;  coordination  was  good,  there  was  a  slight  Romberg 
sign  and  wavering  gait.  He  was  oriented  lor  place  and  persons, 
and  mostly  for  time;  his  general  memory  was  poor  for  both 
recent  and  remote  events;  he  could  not  tell  the  times  and  places 
of  his  occupations;  he  showed  no  insight  into  his  condition  and 
said  he  thought  his  admission  into  the  Hospital  was  a  "frame- 
up"  ;  an  occasional  slight  buzzing  in  his  left  ear  was  the  nearest 
to  an  hallucination  that  was  detected;  and  no  delusions  other 
than  that  recorded  above;  his  general  intelligence  was  poor;  he 
was  irritable,  easily  angered  when  questioned  and  swore 
fluently;  in  general,  he  appeared  to  be  happy;  he  emitted  loud 
screams  occasionally,  but  no  reason  for  these  could  be  obtained ; 
his  writing  was  ataxic,  his  speech  was  tremulous ;  he  became  un- 
tidy in  habits ;  did  not  talk  voluntarily ;  he  became  much  demented 
and  enfeebled.  Death  was  due  to  pulmonary  tuberculosis.  The 
examination  of  the  brain  showed  that  the  convolutions  were 
shrunken  over  the  anterior  half. 

GENERAL  PARALYSIS  OF  THE  INSANE,  DISCUSSION 

Of  these  six  cases,  three  had  atrophies  in  the  frontal  region 
and  three  were  of  the  anterior  portions  of  the  cerebrum.  Only 
one  of  these  has  been  described  as  showing  a  great  degree  of 
atrophy,  viz.,  case  12.  All  of  these  patients  showed  a  very 
great  degree  of  dementia  previous  to  death.  The  one  who 
seemed  least  demented  was  case  13,  although  the  degree  of 
dementia  can  not  be  determined  with  accuracy.  It  is  of  interest 
to  note  that  the  total  duration  of  the  disease  in  this  patient 
was  longer  than  in  the  other  five  patients,  and  the  dementia 


34  SHEPHERD  IVORY  FRANZ 

appeared  to  be  more  gradual  in  its  appearance.  With  this 
case  there  should  be  compared  case  14,  who  lived  less  than 
a  year  after  the  first  signs  of  mental  abnormality.  Most  of 
these  patients  towards  the  end  lived  practically  a  vegetable  ex- 
istence; they  were  oblivious  of  their  surroundings;  they  reacted 
little,  if  at  all,  to  ordinary  stimuli  and  were  apparently  unable 
to  comprehend  anything.  These  general  mental  conditions  which 
are  found  in  all  cases  of  paresis  are  not,  however,  correlated 
with  the  degree  or  with  the  extent  of  the  atrophy  of  the  brain, 
for  the  patient  (case  15)  whose  brain  showed  the  greatest 
amount  of  atrophy  was  no  more  demented  than  the  other  pa- 
tients at  the  time  of  death,  nor  was  the  patient  (case  12)  whose 
brain  showed  the  greatest  degree  of  atrophy  any  more  demented 
than  the  other  patients.  Both  of  these  cases  showed  an 
extreme  degree  of  dementia,  and  in  this  respect  they  differed 
not  at  all  from  the  other  cases  (with  the  possible  exception 
of  case  13,  which  is  mentioned  above). 

It  is  to  be  noted  that  all  of  the  patients  exhibited  motor  de- 
rangements. With  the  exception  of  case  15,  tremors,  especially 
of  the  lips,  tongue  and  hand,  were  found.  Case  15  did  not 
show  tremors  of  these  parts,  although  his  writing  was  tremulous 
and  associated  with  this  there  was  a  certain  degree  of  ataxia. 
It  does  not  appear,  however,  that  the  ataxic  gait  and  the  in- 
ability to  walk  steadily  and  to  perform  other  necessary  move- 
ments were  any  greater  in  those  patients  who  exhibited  lesions 
extending  beyond  the  frontal  lobes,  and  in  this  respect  we  find 
no  definite  correlation  between  the  atrophies  which  included 
the  electrically  excitable  (i.e.  motor)  portions  of  the  cerebrum 
and  those  which  did  not  extend  as  far  backwards.  Only  one 
of  the  six  patients  had  had  convulsions,  case  10,  and  this  pa- 
tient it  will  be  remembered  was  a  case  in  which  the  frontal 
regions  were  atrophied.  On  the  other  hand,  two  of  the  patients 
(cases  10  and  12)  developed  contractures,  and  it  is  of  interest 
to  note  that  both  of  these  were  cases  of  frontal  atrophy.  Periods 
of  confusion  or  of  bewilderment,  which  are  probably  similar  in 
nature  to  the  convulsive  seizures,  were  noted  in  cases  10,  n 
and  12,  and  in  one  of  these  (case  n)  such  a  condition  was  at 


one  time  preceded  by  a  period  of  unconsciousness.  These  three 
cases  were  those  in  which  only  frontal  lesions  were  discovered. 
The  affective  states  in  these  patients  were  not  similar.  In 
general,  although  they  appeared  contented,  some  had  feelings 
of  exaltation,  and  at  times  some  were  depressed.  Cases  10  and 

11  appeared  to  be  almost  continually  contented  and  cheerful. 
Case  15  appeared  to  have  feelings  of  contentment,  of  well-being, 
and  of  self-importance  at  practically  all  times,  and,  although  he 
complained  of  having  been  sent  to  an  institution  and  called  it 
a  "frame-up,"  his  remarks  about  this  matter  appeared  to  have 
little  affective  accompaniment.     On  the  other  hand,  patient  14 
appeared  to  be  indifferent,  and  the  only  evidence  of  worry  or 
depression  on  his  part  was  his  disinclination  to  talk  and  his 
lack  of  cooperation.     The  reasons  for  the  impulsive  tendencies 
which  were  noted  in  his  case  were  not  learned,  but  these  were 
not  accompanied   by  noticeable   emotional   reactions.      Patients 

12  and  13  showed  variations  in  affectivity,  for  they  ranged  from 
sullenness,   moroseness   and   homicidal   tendencies   to  those   of 
cheerfulness  and  exaltation.    It  will  be  observed  that  these  varia- 
tions are  not  associated  with  particular  lesions,  and  that  similar 
variations  in  emotional  or  affective  states  are  found  associated 
with  the  more  extensive  as  well  as  with  the  frontal  atrophies. 

In  none  of  these  patients  were  hallucinations  noted  at  any 
time,  with  the  possible  exception  of  case  15,  who  had  complained 
of  a  buzzing  in  his  ear.  Whether  this  was  due  to  a  peripheral 
or  a  central  irritation  could  not  be  determined;  it  did  not  ap- 
parently lead  to  any  interpretative  elaboration,  and  because  of  this 
it  may  be  disregarded. 

Case  14  showed  no  evidence  of  the  presence  of  a  delusion  at 
any  time.  The  other  five  patients  were  noted  to  have  delusions 
of  grandeur  or  of  persecution,  although  these  were  most  fre- 
quently mild  in  character  and  those  of  some  of  the  patients  were 
not  much  beyond  the  scope  of  the  individual's  life.  In  a  few 
instances  the  delusions  were  of  a  very  fantastic  and  absurd  char- 
acter, notably  those  of  patient  10.  It  appears  remarkable,  how- 
ever, that  with  the  exception  of  the  temporary  and  unsystematized 


36  SHEPHERD  IVORY  FRANZ 

delusions  regarding  the  race  horse  (case  n),  the  possession  of 
houses  and  diamonds  (case  13),  and  some  of  grandeur,  of  which 
the  nature  was  not  specified  (case  15)  and  which  were  noted  only 
previous  to  his  entrance  to  the  Hospital,  these  evidences  of  mental 
abnormality  were  not  prominent  in  these  three  cases.  Moreover, 
it  may  be  mentioned  that  some  of  the  delusions  of  patient  12,  e.g., 
those  which  were  evidenced  by  his  complaints  that  various  people 
were  trying  to  get  his  money  away  from  him,  were  not  too  far 
out  of  bounds,  and  might  not  have  been  delusions  in  the  strict 
sense  of  that  term.  In  this  connection  it  must  also  be  remem- 
bered that  with  the  possible  exception  of  the  delusion  regarding 
his  retention  in  the  Hospital,  which  he  described  as  a  "frame-up," 
patient  15  did  not  show  any  evidence  of  delusions  during  his 
Hospital  residence  of  about  eighteen  months.  We  are  led  to  con- 
clude from  the  facts  which  have  been  recorded  that  these  mental 
symptoms  do  not  appear  to  be  concomitants  of  special  extent,  or 
of  special  degree,  of  atrophy  of  the  anterior  regions  of  the  brain, 
and  that  delusions,  in  cases  of  paresis  at  least,  may  not  be  due  to 
the  cerebral  changes  in  the  frontal  lobes. 

It  is  of  interest  to  note  that  in  these  cases  the  memory  dif- 
fered very  greatly.  Although  the  memory  of  all  was  very  poor 
during  the  later  weeks  or  months  of  their  Hospital  life,  in  some 
cases,  even  at  times  when  the  other  evidences  of  abnormality 
were  plain,  e.g.,  delusions,  it  was  noted  that  the  memory  for 
remote  and  recent  events  was  fair.  Thus,  case  10  appeared  to 
have  a  remarkably  good  memory  for  the  events  of  his  past  life 
and  even  for  other  events  which  did  not  so  immediately  concern 
him,  although  his  delusions  regarding  his  wealth  were  very  ab- 
surd and  in  this  respect  his  judgment  was  very  defective.  Re- 
garding case  14,  no  information  could  be  obtained  on  account  of 
his  lack  of  cooperation.  The  memory  in  the  other  four  cases 
may  be  said  to  correspond  in  general  with  the  degree  of  de- 
mentia, although  not  with  the  extent  or  the  degree  of  the  atrophy. 

In  most  cases  the  degree  of  orientation  corresponded  also  with 
the  degree  of  dementia,  and  also  with  the  degree  of  memory. 
Patient  10,  who  recalled  quite  well  both  recent  and  remote  events, 


37 

was  fully  oriented  for  time,  place  and  persons,  and  in  general  the 
other  patients  who  exhibited  memory  defects  were  not  oriented. 
One  exception  to  this  general  statement  is  patient  15,  who,  al- 
though he  could  not  recall  much  of  his  own  personal  history,  was 
well  oriented  for  place  and  for  persons  and  also  exhibited  fair 
orientation  for  time.  It  may  also  be  noted  that  patient  14,  whose 
lack  of  cooperation  has  been  mentioned,  appeared  to  know  where 
he  was. 

At  the  time  of  their  admission  to  the  Hospital  the  attention  of 
these  patients  could  be  readily  obtained,  and,  in  two  cases,  10 
and  13,  it  was  noted  that  they  could  attend  to  things  well.  The 
latter  patients  understood  what  was  said  to  them  and  could  carry 
on  a  conversation  coherently  and  relevantly,  and  appeared  to  be 
able  to  attend  to  such  impressions  as  they  received.  Cases  n 
and  15  also  exhibited  a  fair  degree  of  attention  ability,  although 
at  the  same  time  it  was  noted  that  their  memory  was  defective. 
On  the  other  hand,  case  12  was  apparently  unable  to  attend  to 
things  well,  for  'he  is  noted  to  have  been  unable  to  attend  to 
what  was  said  to  him  or  to  comprehend  readily,  and  case  14  also 
exhibited  a  similar  difficulty  of  attention.  These  variations,  like 
those  in  the  dementia  precox  group,  are  not  associated  with  the 
special  lesions. 

These  six  cases  of  paresis  do  not  differ  extensively  from  one 
another  in  symptomatology,  although  they  may  be  differentiated 
as  belonging  to  the  simple  dementing  form  (e.g.,  case  14)  or  to 
the  expansive  form  (e.g.,  case  10)  of  the  disease.  Although  the 
symptoms  in  these  cases  are  more  nearly  equal  than  in  the  cases 
of  dementia  precox,  the  differences  are  also  sufficiently  evident  to 
make  possible  a  comparison  with  the  difference  in  the  extent  of 
the  lesions.  It  appears,  however,  that  the  symptomatological  dif- 
ferences are  not  to  be  correlated  with  the  anatomical  differences 
in  the  extent  or  the  severity  of  the  anterior  atrophies.  Those 
patients  who  exhibited  only  frontal  atrophies  were,  apparently, 
as  markedly  demented,  they  showed  about  the  same  degree  of 
emotional  or  affective  change,  and  they  exhibited  lack  of  memory, 
of  orientation,  of  attention,  and  of  comprehension  to  about  the 


38  SHEPHERD  IVORY  FRANZ 

same  degree  as  those  cases  in  which  the  atrophy  extended  beyond 
the  limits  of  the  frontal  lobes.  On  the  other  hand,  it  appears 
equally  probable  that  with  frontal  or  with  anterior  atrophies  some 
paretic  parents  will  exhibit  a  fair  degree  of  memory  and  others 
an  almost  complete  loss  of  memory,  that  some  will  have  fantastic 
or  persecutory  or  grandiose  delusions  and  others  will  have  none  of 
these.  It  is  apparent,  therefore,  that  with  comparable  lesions 
the  symptoms  may  differ,  and  that  with  lesions  which  do  not 
even  approximately  correspond  the  symptoms  may  be  similar.. 


ARTERIOSCLEROTIC  DEMENTIA,  CLINICAL 
HISTORIES 

Case  1 6,  white  male,  was  admitted  at  the  age  of  63,  and  died 
7  years  and  4  months  later. 

The  diagnosis  on  admission  was  acute  confusional  insanity, 
intoxication  psychosis;  the  duration  was  given  as  three  months 
plus;  he  was  noted  as  having  used  alcohol  only  moderately;  the 
medical  certificate  stated  that  he  burned  papers  on  the  floor 
of  his  room;  frequently  wandered  around  aimlessly  at  night; 
showed  loss  of  memory,  with  all  the  symptoms  of  senility;  had 
fixed  transitory  delusions,  one  of  his  fixed  delusions  being  that 
he  was  "still  in  active  service  in  the  army,  but  at  that  time  on 
furlough";  he  was  disoriented  for  time;  he  made  meaningless 
remarks.  His  family  history  was  negative.  On  admission  he 
was  disoriented  for  time;  appeared  perfectly  satisfied  with  his 
surroundings;  showed  only  a  slight  amount  of  insight;  he  was 
up  and  about  the  ward ;  his  expression  was  placid ;  he  was  poorly 
nourished  but  there  were  no  physical  or  neurological  abnormali- 
ties. His  intellect  and  memory  were  defective;  his  attention 
could  be  obtained  and  held  without  difficulty;  he  was  quiet  and 
orderly;  he  appeared  to  be  much  demented,  and  seldom  spoke 
unless  addressed;  he  remained  seated  in  one  place  in  the  ward 
the  greater  part  of  the  day;  he  comprehended  readily,  and  his. 
replies  were  given  promptly  and  were  relevant  and  coherent; 
no  delusions  or  hallucinations  were  elicited.  Later  he  claimed 
that  some  one  put  laudanum  in  his  beer,  and  that  this  had 
doped  him  and  caused  him  to  be  sent  here;  when  asked  if  any 
one  had  put  poison  in  his  food  since  he  was  in  the  Hospital, 
he  said  it  was  not  for  him  to  say,  he  became  indignant,  his  face 
flushed,  and  he  said  he  had  a  right  to  his  beliefs;  a  speech 
defect  was  observed.  For  over  a  year  previous  to  his  death 
he  was  confined  to  bed;  he  became  disturbed  and  excited  if 
he  did  not  get  the  things  he  wanted;  he  thought  some  one  had 


40  SHEPHERD  IVORY  FRANZ 

stolen  his  money,  hidden  his  clothes,  and  imposed  upon  him  in 
various  other  ways;  his  memory  grew  more  defective,  and  he 
was  unable  to  give  the  name  of  the  institution ;  he  thought  some 
one  was  sending  electric  currents  through  him,  and  that  these 
caused  contracture  of  his  fingers;  his  feet  and  legs  became  con- 
tracted about  six  months  before  his  death. 

The  autopsy  showed  atrophy  of  the  frontal  convolutions;  there 
was  left  bronchopneumonia,  and  the  kidneys  showed  a  slight 
degree  of  swelling  and  some  inflammation. 

Case  17,  white  male,  was  admitted  at  the  age  of  65  and 
subsequently  lived  for  a  little  over  a  year. 

His  family  and  personal  history  was  negative;  the  medical 
certificate  stated  that  the  patient  had  been  nervous  and  talked 
at  random  about  large  amounts  of  money  he  thought  he  was 
going  to  get  from  people  in  England;  he  made  all  kinds  of 
absurd  statements ;  he  asked  to  have  his  ,f eet  and  toes  amputated ; 
he  could  not  carry  on  a  connected  conversation  and  he  attempted 
violence  upon  other  inmates  in  the  Soldiers'  Home  in  which 
he  was.  On  admission  to  the  Hospital  he  was  restless  most 
of  the  time;  he  would  not  sit  for  even  a  few  minutes  in  one 
place;  he  appeared  to  be  profoundly  demented;  he  was  disori- 
ented; his  memory  was  markedly  defective,  and  his  talk  was 
disconnected  and  incoherent ;  coarse  tremors  of  the  tongue  were 
noted;  the  pupils  reacted  slowly  in  accommodation,  and  the 
superficial  reflexes  could  not  be  elicited;  his  coordination  was 
fair;  physically  he  showed  signs  of  cardiovascular  renal  disease 
and  pulmonary  tuberculosis. 

In  addition  to  these  the  autopsy  showed  marked  shrinkage 
of  the  brain,  especially  in  the  frontal  regions. 

Case  1 8,  colored  male,  73  years  old  at  the  time  of  admission, 
lived  for  about  I  year  subsequently. 

The  certificate  accompanying  the  patient  stated  that  he  had 
impaired  cerebration,  and  for  the  preceding  six  years  there  had 
been  a  tendency  on  his  part  to  wander  away.  On  his  admission 


SYMPTOMATOLOGICAL  DIFFERENCES  IN  THE  INSANE      41 

to  the  Hospital  he  was  found  to  be  enfeebled;  he  was  restless; 
the  only  thing  that  could  be  elicited  from  him  by  questioning 
was  his  name;  he  was  disoriented;  he  talked  very  indistinctly; 
he  seemed  to  be  very  much  demented;  he  was  tidy  in  habits; 
spasmodic  laughing  and  crying  and  negativistic  tendencies  were 
noted;  he  showed  perseveration ;  his  voluntary  acts  were  appar- 
ently purposeless,  and  his  movements  were  inaccurate;  neuro- 
logically,  there  were  no  atrophies  found;  a  slight  arcus  senilis 
was  present;  there  were  marked  tremors;  his  gait  was  slow  and 
unsteady,  and  his  walk  was  shuffling;  he  was  noted  to  have  some 
ataxia;  although  a  right  homonymous  hemiamopia  was  noted 
in  the  history,  this  was  not  detectable  at  a  later  date  and  may 
have  been  surmised  because  of  special  acts  due  to  his  negativism 
and  his  general  mental  inertia;  sensory  and  motor  aphasia  were 
also  noted;  smell  was  diminished;  there  was  a  diminished  sense 
of  temperature  and  a  hypoalgesia;  he  was  untidy  in  habits  and 
he  could  not  find  his  bed ;  he  took  no  interest  in  his  surroundings, 
and  at  the  time  of  his  death  he  was  in  a  condition  of  extreme 
dementia. 

The  autopsy  revealed  shrinkage  of  the  cerebral  convolutions, 
especially  in  the  frontal  lobes,  pachymeningitis,  slight  arterio- 
sclerosis of  the  basal  vessels,  valvular  heart  disease,  atheromatous 
aorta,  congestion  of  the  kidneys. 

Case  19,  white  male,  was  admitted  at  the  age  of  62  and 
lived  1 6  years.  At  the  time  of  admission  no  information  was 
received  regarding  him  except  that  the  diagnosis  of  "acute  mania" 
had  been  made. 

In  the  Hospital  he  was  orderly  and  quiet  and  assisted  with 
the  ward  work;  he  was  fond  of  reading;  he  spoke  very  little 
voluntarily,  but  answered  questions  readily;  he  was  untidy  in 
appearance,  and  he  collected  and  filled  his  bed  with  trash;  he 
was  very  childish;  at  times  he  talked  almost  incessantly.  Later 
he  was  noisy  and  restless,  somewhat  destructive  and  untidy,  and 
he  became  hypochondriacal,  and  sometimes  had  to  be  restrained 
but  at  other  times  he  associated  with  the  other  patients  and 
played  games  with  them;  he  comprehended  what  was  said  to 


42  SHEPHERD  IVORY  FRANZ 

him;  he  was  coherent  and  relevant  in  his  conversation,  but  his 
reasoning  and  judgment  were  limited;  his  memory  was  poor 
for  recent  events,  but  he  appeared  to  take  considerable  interest 
in  his  surroundings,  and  was  oriented  in  all  spheres ;  his  station 
was  good  but  his  gait  was  rather  unsteady;  there  was  noted 
a  slight  double  cataract  and  double  arcus  senilis. 

The  autopsy  revealed  internal  hemorrhagic  pachymeningitis, 
shrinkage  of  the  anterior  convolutions  but  no  other  gross  cerebral 
lesions,  sclerosis  of  the  aorta  and  of  the  cerebral  basal 
vessels,  pulmonary  tuberculosis  and  pneumonia,  and  intestinal 
tuberculosis. 

Case  20,  white  male,  age  72  on  admission  and  lived  for  i 
year  and  %  month.  He  had  been  gradually  losing  his  mental 
faculties  for  a  year,  and  while  in  another  institution  he  had 
been  unable  to  find  his  bed  and  to  take  care  of  himself;  he  was 
disoriented  and  confused,  and  had  no  insight. 

There  was  fine  tremor  of  the  tongue;  pupils  were  slightly 
irregular  in  outline;  the  heart  was  somewhat  enlarged;  there 
was  a  coarse  tremor  of  the  right  arm ;  the  tendon  reflexes  could 
not  be  elicited ;  station  and  gait  were  greatly  impaired.  He  was  not 
oriented  for  time,  place,  or  person;  his  memory  for  recent  and 
remote  events  was  very  poor;  emotionally  he  was  apparently 
indifferent;  there  was  no  insight;  no  hallucinations  or  delusions 
were  detected;  the  patient  was  quiet,  feeble,  and  was  agreeable 
and  rather  childish  in  his  reaction;  three  or  four  days  before 
his  death  he  could  not  move  his  left  arm  and  leg;  the  tongue 
was  protuded  slightly  to  the  left ;  there  was  Babinski  phenonema 
on  the  left;  whether  or  not  this  attack  was  accompanied  by  a 
convulsive  seizure  is  unknown. 

The  autopsy  showed  shrinkage  of  the  frontal  lobes;  hem- 
orrhage was  found  in  the  Rolandic  region;  in  the  left  occipital 
lobe  there  was  an  old  softening  affecting  this  part;  the  heart 
valves  were  insufficient,  and  the  heart  enlarged;  and 
parenchymatous  nephritis. 


SYMPTOM ATOLOGICAL  DIFFERENCES  IN  THE  INSANE      43 

Case  21,  colored  male,  65  years  old  at  the  time  of  admission 
and  lived  3*4  years  subsequently. 

The  medical  certificate  stated  that  the  patient  had  been  normal 
until  a  few  years  previous  to  his  commitment ;  he  had  bad  mem- 
ory and  did  not  seem  to  remember  anything;  he  was  disoriented 
for  time  and  place;  he  was  restless,  he  talked  senselessly;  his 
reasoning  was  bad ;  and  he  appeared  to  be  excited.  The  physical 
examination  showed  bronchitis,  sclerotic  superficial  vessels,  feeble 
heart  sounds,  no  tremors,  coordination  was  good ;  he  had  a  poor 
grip;  there  was  a  slight  arcus  senilis  with  some  visual  impair- 
ment; no  speech  defect  was  observed.  On  admission  he  appeared 
to  be  senile;  he  was  quiet  and  tractable;  he  showed  some  con- 
fusion and  marked  clouding  of  consciousness ;  he  was  disoriented ; 
his  attention  could  not  be  held;  his  memory  was  poor;  his 
conversation  was  rambling  and  incoherent,  and  he  usually 
answered  incorrectly;  no  definite  evidence  of  hallucinations  or 
delusions  was  obtained,  but  he  appeared  to  be  suspicious  that 
he  was  to  be  harmed  in  some  manner;  at  one  time,  however,  he 
said  that  colored  people  had  been  after  him  and  almost  scared 
him  to  death,  but  that  white  people  came  to  his  rescue;  and 
later  he  also  spoke  of  his  farm  and  sheep  having  been  stolen, 
etc. ;  at  times  he  was  restless  and  wandered  around  aimlessly, 
talking  in  a  rambling  manner;  he  became  untidy  in  habits;  he 
was  very  restless,  and  asked  that  he  be  permitted  to  go  to  his 
work. 

The  autopsy  revealed  atrophy  of  the  anterior  convolutions, 
sclerosis  of  the  basal  arteries,  dilated  heart,  and  chronic  cystitis. 

Case  22,  white  male,  was  70  years  of  age  on  admission  and 
lived  for  only  i  month  subsequently. 

His  mother  had  been  insane  and  confined  in  a  hospital ;  other- 
wise, his  family  history  was  negative.  The  patient  always  had 
been  a  heavy  drinker  and  occasionally  he  had  a  prolonged  period 
of  intoxication;  he  had  been  treated  for  dipsomania  a  number 
of  times  and  six  months  prior  to  his  entrance  to  the  Hospital, 
following  a  lengthened  drinking  spree,  he  became  quarrelsome 
and  abusive;  he  thought  other  men  were  putting  lice  into  his 


44  SHEPHERD  IVORY  FRANZ 

bed  and  trying  to  injure  his  reputation  in  other  ways;  he  threat- 
ened them;  he  believed  he  was  in  command  and  ordered  those 
about  him  to  do  things  for  him;  he  heard  voices  of  women  and 
of  his  superior  officers  talking  to  him  and  he  replied  in  a  loud 
boisterous  voice.  Physically  his  muscles  were  of  poor  tone; 
his  coordination  was  fair;  his  station  and  gait  were  unsteady, 
and  his  apparent  delusion  regarding  lice  may  be  explained  by 
the  fact  that  these  vermin  were  actually  found  upon  his  body; 
he  was  neat  and  tidy  in  appearance;  he  was  disoriented;  he  had 
no  insight;  his  general  memory  was  fair;  there  were  auditory 
hallucinations  of  voices,  but  he  would  give  little  information 
about  the  voices,  people,  or  what  was  said  to  him;  he  became 
irascible;  swore  frequently  and  fluently;  he  thought  everything 
in  the  ward  belonged  to  him ;  he  gave  orders  to  others  to  attend 
to  the  horses,  etc. ;  and  he  conversed  with  imaginary  people  out 
of  the  window. 

The  autopsy  showed  that  the  convolutions  were  atrophied 
anteriorly ;  there  were  also  atheroma  of  the  aorta,  cystic  kidneys, 
interstitial  nephritis,  and  cystitis. 

Case  23,  white  male,  was  admitted  at  the  age  of  47  and 
lived  i  year  and  7  months  subsequently. 

At  the  time  of  his  admission  no  medical  certificate  was 
received  and  no  previous  family  history  was  obtained;  at  first 
he  was  diagnosed  as  an  unclassified  psychosis,  but  on  account 
of  the  pathological  findings  this  was  changed  to  arteriosclerotic 
dementia.  On  admission  he  appeared  to  have  no  use  of  his 
legs  from  his  knees  downward;  he  said  he  knew  he  was  going 
to  be  killed  and  burning  was  the  way  to  do  it;  he  imagined 
that  people  were  talking  about  him,  and  he  heard  them  talking 
about  it  at  night;  he  imagined  he  had  plenty  of  money;  he  was 
noisy,  untidy,  occasionally  talkative;  he  continually  complained 
about  everything,  and  he  was  very  irritable;  he  was  oriented; 
his  memory  was  only  fair;  at  times  he  was  stupid;  at  one  time 
he  had  a  convulsion  affecting  the  right  side,  after  which  he 
could  not  talk.  Later  he  had  a  convulsion  principally  on  the 
left  which  also  affected  the  right. 


SYMPTOM ATOLOGICAL  DIFFERENCES  IN  THE  INSANE      45 

At  the  autopsy  it  was  found  that  he  had  bronchopneumonia, 
atheroma  of  the  coronary  artery  and  of  the  aorta,  nephritis, 
and  the  convolutions  of  the  anterior  half  of  the  brain  were 
considerably  shrunken. 

Case  24,  white  male,  admitted  to  the  Hospital  at  the  age  of 
55,  and  lived  nearly  18  years  subsequently.  He  had  been  ad- 
mitted to  a  Soldiers'  Home  because  of  a  paralysis  and  epileptic 
seizures.  There  he  attempted  suicide  and  was  violent  at  times. 

On  entrance  to  the  Hospital  he  was  found  to  be  deaf;  he 
was  depressed  and  had  many  morbid  fancies.  Later  he  was  noted 
to  be  feeble,  quiet,  orderly;  he  did  not  answer  intelligently;  his 
mind  wandered;  he  was  indifferent  to  his  surroundings;  he  was 
completely  disoriented  and  showed  no  insight  into  his  condition ; 
at  times  he  became  very  talkative  and  destructive,  although  most 
frequently  he  was  quiet  and  comfortable;  delusions  of  a  pleas- 
urable nature  were  evident  at  times,  and  later  these  gave  way 
to  delusions  of  persecution  when  he  talked- about  the  property 
and  money  which  had  been  stolen  from  him;  there  was  left 
hemiplegia  with  contractures ;  his  gait  was  firm  and  rugged 
for  one  of  his  physical  condition,  but  he  dragged  his  left  leg; 
the  knee  jerks  were  absent. 

The  autopsy  showed  slight  enlargement  of  the  heart,  hypostatic 
congestion  of  the  lungs,  peritonitis,  ulcerative  cholecystitis, 
granular  kidneys,  marked  shrinkage  of  the  brain,  but  no  areas  of 
softening  or  of  hemorrhage  could  be  found  to  account  for  the 
hemiplegia. 

ARTERIOSCLEROTIC  DEMENTIA,  DISCUSSION 

Only  two  of  these  cases  (16  and  20)  are  strictly  frontal  cases. 
Two  others  (17  and  18)  were  cases  in  which  there  was  a  certain 
degree  of  atrophy  of  the  cerebrum  as  a  whole  in  addition  to 
the  marked  frontal  atrophy.  The  remaining  five  cases  were 
those  in  which  the  atrophy  extended  over  the  whole  anterior 
portions  of  the  cerebrum.  Case  22  was  noted  to  be  an  alcoholic 
case,  but  the  relation  of  alcoholism  to  the  mental  disorder  is 
not  clear,  and  the  facts  which  have  been  recorded  in  the  history 


46  SHEPHERD  IVORY  FRANZ 

do  not  give  any  indication  that  alcohol  played  any  great  part 
in  the  production  of  the  symptoms  during  his  Hospital  resi- 
dence. We  may  believe,  however,  that  it  was  a  contributing 
factor.  Case  24  appears  at  first  sight  from  the  history  to  be  a 
case  complicated  with  epilepsy,  but  no  epileptiform  convulsions 
occurred  during  the  period  of  his  Hospital  residence,  and  it 
seems  more  likely  that  the  convulsions  were  of  a  character  which 
later  resulted  in  the  left  hemiplegia,  in  other  words,  that  they 
were  seizures  similar  to  an  apoplexy.  Before  death  all  of  these 
patients  exhibited  plain  evidence  of  dementia.  They  were  en- 
feebled, childish,  indifferent,  confused,  were  not  affected  by 
their  surroundings  to  any  extnet,  and  most  often  exhibited  no 
insight  into  their  mental  condition,  etc.  The  two  cases  which 
showed  most  evidence  of  mental  enfeeblement  (cases  17  and  18) 
were  those  in  which  some  general  atrophy  was  found  to  ac- 
company the  intensive  frontal  atrophies,  and  it  is  possible  that 
the  great  degree  of  dementia  in  these  cases  is  to  be  correlated 
with  the  extension  of  the  atrophy  to  the  posterior  regions  of  the 
cerebrum,  although  the  atrophy  in  the  latter  areas  was  not  of 
great  degree.  The  other  seven  cases  did  not  differ  to  a  great 
extent  in  the  apparent  degree  of  dementia  which  was  present 
immediately  preceding  death,  and  in  this  respect  there  can  be  no 
great  degree  of  correlation  between  the  extent  of  the  atrophy 
and  the  degree  of  the  dementia. 

The  physical  enfeeblement  probably  kept  most  of  these  patients 
less  active  than  would  have  been  normal  for  them,  and  the  two 
patients  who  exhibited  effects  of  paralysis,  cases  16  and  24,  were 
especially  orderly  and  quiet  most  of  the  time.  The  physical 
enfeeblement  did  not,  however,  prevent  these  patients  from  be- 
coming disturbed,  noisy,  restless  and  at  times  excited,  and  these 
states  alternated  with  states  of  quiescence  and  even  confusion, 
the  only  exception  being  that  case  20  (with  frontal  atrophy) 
was  uniformly  apparently  indifferent,  inactive  and  agreeable. 
Lesions  to  account  for  the  paralytic  phenomena  were  not  dis- 
covered, and  it  may  be  that  part  of  the  contractures  in  these 
patients  were  due  to  disuse  and  not  to  special  paralysis.  In 
other  respects  every  one  of  these  patients  showed  variations 


SYMPTOM ATOLOGICAL  DIFFERENCES  IN  THE  INSANE      47 

from  the  normal  motor  ability,  both  in  the  presence  of  tremors 
and  in  their  inaccurate,  slow,  and  feeble  voluntary  movements. 
Patient  18,  whose  brain  showed  some  general  atrophy  in  addition 
to  the  marked  atrophy  in  tjje  frontal  regions,  also  exhibited 
negativistic  tendencies,  and  at  other  times  he  appeared  to  be 
purposelessly  active.  This  patient,  it  will  be  noted,  also  showed 
other  symptoms  like  those  of  the  dementia  precox  group  in  that 
he  was  noted  to  laugh  and  to  cry  spasmodically  and  without 
apparent  reason  and  to  show  perseveration.  Both  the  similarities 
and  the  variations  in  these  motor  symptoms  in  these  patients  do 
not  appear  to  be  directly  correlated  with  the  distribution  of  the 
atrophies,  for  the  unsteadiness  and  the  tremors  and  the  rest- 
lessness were  found  equally  in  those  with  frontal  and  in  those 
with  the  more  extensive  atrophies. 

The  speech  disturbances  were  not  as  pronounced  as  in  the 
cases  of  general  paralysis  of  the  insane.  At  least  four  of  these 
cases  could  talk  readily,  and  also  coherently  and  relevantly,  and 
there  was  no  difficulty  in  carrying  on  a  conversation  with  them, 
if  one  did  not  approach  their  delusions  or  in  the  conversation 
make  too  much  of  a  demand  to  strain  their  memory.  There 
was  in  general  none  of  the  speech  defects  which  are  so  com- 
monly found  in  paretics,  and  when  the  patients  would  and  could 
talk,  their  words  were  usually  well-pronounced.  Defective  speech 
was,  however,  noted  in  some  of  them;  patient  16  was  observed 
to  have  a  slight  speech  defect;  patient  18  was  also  recorded 
at  one  time  to  have  both  sensory  and  motor  aphasia,  but  the 
diagnosis  of  this  condition  is  a  very  doubtful  one,  and  should 
be  accepted,  if  at  all,  with  caution,  for  at  that  time  the  patient 
was  in  a  very  negativistic  mood ;  patient  23  also  showed  a  motor 
aphasia  after  a  convulsive  attack.  Assuming  the  accuracy  of 
the  facts  just  mentioned,  we  may  believe  that  these  conditions 
were  due  to  local  cerebral  injuries  that  were,  however,  not 
discovered  at  autopsy,  and  which  may  have  been  temporary 
or  functional.  Considering  only  the  speech  ability  anterior  to 
these  accidents,  we  find  that,  with  the  exception  of  case  23, 
the  speech  of  all  the  patients  was  similar ;  at  times  it  was  almost 
incessant,  at  other  times  they  conversed  voluntarily  very  little 


48  SHEPHERD  IVORY  FRANZ 

but  would  answer  questions,  and  at  other  times  it  was  difficult 
to  get  any  information  from  them.  In  other  respects  also  their 
talk  was  similar;  two  patients  with  frontal  and  two  patients 
with  anterior  atrophies  talked  at  random,  or  incoherently,  two 
other  patients  with  frontal  and  two  patients  with  anterior 
atrophies  talked  relevantly  and  could  carry  on  conversations  on 
minor  matters.  In  these  respects,  therefore,  we  find  with  similar 
atrophies  quite  dissimilar  characters  of  symptoms,  and  with 
dissimilar  atrophies  quite  similar  symptoms. 

The  affective  conditions  in  these  patients  differed  widely.  Of 
the  patients  with  frontal  lesions  three  varied  from  an  indifference 
to  a  general  satisfied  condition,  but  the  fourth  patient  exhibited 
from  time  to  time  spasmodic  laughing  and  crying,  but  whether 
this  alternation  was  accompanied  by  the  appropriate  emotional 
states  is  not  sure.  This  reaction  has  previously  been  compared 
with  those  of  dementia  precox  patients,  and  in  addition  it  may 
be  said  that  it  also  resembles  to  a  certain  degree  the  similar 
symptom  associated  with  lesions  of  the  optic  thalamus,  especially 
in  view  of  the  association  of  hypoesthesia  for  temperature  and 
pain.  One  of  these  patients  (case  16),  in  speaking  of  his  de- 
lusions of  persecution,  did  not  appear  to  react  with  appropriate 
emotional  tone  to  them.  The  other  five  patients,  those  with 
anterior  lesions,  were  depressed,  suspicious,  irascible,  and  quarrel- 
some, with  more  normal  or  more  contented,  or  indifferent, 
intervening  periods.  Four  of  the  latter  group  (patients  21,  22, 
23,  and  24)  had  corresponding  delusions,  and  their  affective 
states  were  associated  with  and  corresponded  with  these  other 
mental  derangements.  Patient  19,  on  the  other  hand,  appeared 
hypochondriacal  without  apparent  reason,  for  if  he  had  delu- 
sions, they  were  not  made  evident  (see  below),  and  no  hallucina- 
tions were  discovered.  Whether  or  not  the  greater  frequency 
of  particular  types  of  emotional  reactions  and  of  special  feelings 
is  to  be  definitely  correlated  with  the  more  extensive  lesions  can 
not  be  determined.  That  the  general  atrophy  in  cases  17  and 
1 8,  which  it  will  be  remembered  was  associated  with  a  more 
marked  degree  of  frontal  atrophy,  did  not  produce  similar 
affective  states  is  an  indication  that  the  special  emotional  con- 


SYMPTOM ATOLOGICAL  DIFFERENCES  IN  THE  INSANE      49 

ditions,  which  superficially  appear  to  be  definitely  associated 
with  the  anterior  lesions,  are  better  interpreted  as  chance  oc- 
currences, and  that  in  a  more  extensive  series  they  would  probably 
not  be  found  in  as  great  proportion.  This  conclusion  also  appears 
more  likely  in  view  of  the  results  which  have  been  found  in  the 
other  diseases  which  have  been  studied. 

Only  one-third  of  these  cases  gave  evidence  of  hallucinations. 
These  were  cases  16,  22  and  23.  Apparently  case  16  had 
paresthesias  in  the  fingers,  for  he  complained  that  electric  cur- 
rents were  sent  through  him  so  that  they  caused  contractures 
of  these  parts.  Regarding  the  other  delusions  of  which  he  com- 
plained, viz.,  that  he  had  been  doped  and  that  laudanum  had 
been  put  into  his  beer,  it  is  more  difficult  to  judge  whether 
these  were  purely  ideational  delusions  or  delusions  due  to 
paresthesias.  The  auditory  hallucinations  of  patients  22  and  23 
were  very  evident,  but  when  patient  22  conversed  with  imaginary 
people  outside  of  his  window,  it  was  not  certain  that  the  hallucina- 
tions were  entirely  auditory.  The  peculiar  requests  of  patient 
17  that  his  feet  be  amputated  might  be  due  to  hallucinations, 
but  this  was  not  determined.  Those  patients  who  had  sensory 
defects  (patient  24  with  deafness,  patient  21  with  visual  defect, 
and  patient  18  with  hypoesthesia  for  smell,  temperature  and  pain) 
did  not  apparently  have  hallucinations  either  in  these  fields  or 
in  others,  nor  did  patient  20  in  whose  brain  a  small  softening 
was  discovered  in  the  left  occipital  lobe.  These  facts  do  not 
indicate  any  definite  relation  between  the  mental  conditions  and 
the  extent  or  degree  of  the  cerebral  atrophy. 

There  appears  to  be  no  more  direct  connection  between  the 
presence  or  the  character  of  delusions  and  the  atrophies  of  the 
anterior  portions  of  the  cerebrum.  Patients  18  and  20  did  not 
have  delusions  which  were  detected,  and  one  of  these  (case  18) 
will  be  recalled  as  having  had  general  as  well  as  the  well-marked 
frontal  atrophy,  the  other  being  a  simple  frontal  case.  The 
other  two  patients  with  frontal  atrophies  did  have  delusions, 
some  of  the  delusions  in  both  being  allied  to  hallucinatory 
phenomena,  although  a  conclusion  regarding  this  relation  can 
not  be  stated  with  definiteness.  The  delusion  of  patient  16  that 


50  SHEPHERD  IVORY  FRANZ 

he  was  still  in  active  army  service  was  probably  a  filling-out  due  to 
memory  defects,  but  those  regarding  the  loss  of  money,  the  ac- 
tion of  electric  currents  and  the  presence  of  poison  in  his  food 
are  probably  not  due  to  this  failure  of  memory,  nor  do  the 
delusions  of  patient  17  regarding  the  money  which  he  expected 
from  England  appear  to  be  restrospective  interpretations. 
It  can  not  be  said  with  certainty  that  patient  19  did  or  did  not 
have  delusions;  the  fact  that  he  collected  and  attempted  to  fill 
his  bed  with  trash  indicates  that  he  believed  this  to  have  some 
value  or  to  have  some  relation  to  himself,  but  information  on 
this  point  is  totally  lacking.  The  other  four  patients,  all  with 
anterior  lesions,  had  delusions  of  persecution  which  with  the 
exception  of  those  of  patient  21  alternated  with  mild  delusions 
of  grandeur. 

All  of  these  patients  showed  defective  memory,  and  although 
it  is  almost  impossible  to  determine  the  degree  of  the  defect 
in  the  individual  cases,  the  general  reactions  of  patients  16,  17,  20 
and  21  would  lead  to  the  conclusion  that  they  were  more  abnormal 
in  this  respect  than  the  other  four  cases.  Four  of  these  patients, 
it  will  be  noted,  are  cases  of  frontal  lesions,  and  the  memory 
defects  seem  to  be  correlated  with  the  degree  of  dementia,  since 
it  has  already  been  mentioned  that  two  of  these  (cases  17  and 
18)  were  profoundly  demented.  The  other  two  frontal  cases, 
patients  16  and  20,  were  confused  and  bewildered  and  in  their 
cases  the  memory  defects  may  be  either  evidence  of  the  con- 
fusion or  the  result  or  the  concomitant  of  these  conditions. 

In  marked  contrast  to  the  character  of  the  memory  is  the 
orientation  of  the  individual  cases.  Patients  17,  18,  20,  22,  and 
24  were  completely  disoriented;  patient  16  was  disoriented  for 
time;  and  patient  21  was  disoriented  for  time  and  places.  The 
partial  orientation  of  patients  16  and  21  is  the  more  remarkable 
in  view  of  their  poor  memory,  and  the  good  orientation  of 
patients  19  and  23  is  also  to  be  contrasted  with  the  degree  of 
memory  loss.  The  latter  two  cases,  it  will  be  recalled,  are  cases 
of  anterior  lesions,  and  this  fact  indicates  that  even  with  such 
extensive  changes  orientation  for  time  and  space  may  be  retained. 

Not  much  more  can  be  said  regarding  the  other  mental  pro- 


SYMPTOM ATOLOGICAL  DIFFERENCES  IN  THE  INSANE      51 

cesses  directly,  although  indirect  information  is  obtainable  in 
the  case  histories  regarding  the  patients'  ability  to  attend  to 
impressions.  In  general,  in  these  cases  of  arteriosclerotic  de- 
mentia there  remains  considerable  ability  to  attend  to  impressions, 
as  has  also  been  shown  experimentally,  and  this  ability  appears 
to  be  independent  of  the  memory  disorder.  Even  though  the 
patient  was  apparently  very  much  demented  or  even  confused, 
his  attention  could  usually  be  obtained  without  difficulty,  and 
this  was  noted  especially  for  cases  16,  19,  20,  22,  and  24.  At 
times  patient  23  appeared  stupid,  i.e.,  his  attention  could  not  be 
obtained,  and  although  the  attention  of  patient  21  could  be 
obtained  it  could  not  be  held.  Whether  or  not  we  shall  interpret 
the  perseveration  of  patient  18  as  "good"  or  "poor"  attention 
can  not  be  definitely  settled;  there  is  sufficient  ground  for  either 
conclusion.  The  fact  that  with  either  frontal  or  anterior 
atrophies  there  may  be  good  ability  to  attend  is  an  argument 
against  the  supposition  of  an  "attention  function"  for  the  frontal 
lobes,  and  the  fact  that  the  ease  or  difficulty  of  attracting  the 
attention  did  not  differ  in  accordance  with  the  extent  of  the 
lesions  which  are  here  considered  is  evidence  that  direct  correla- 
tion does  not  exist 

The  variations  in  the  mental  processes  which  have  here  been 
considered  permit  the  conclusion  that  neither  the  extent  nor  the 
severity  of  the  atrophy  of  the  anterior  regions  of  the  brain 
in  arteriosclerotic  dementia  is  directly  correlated  with  the  mental 
symptoms,  and  they  also  show  that  with  similar  atrophies  dis- 
similar symptoms  may  be  frequently  encountered  in  different 
patients. 


SENILE  DEMENTIA,  CLINICAL  HISTORIES 

Case  25,  white  male,  aged  63  on  admission,  had  been  admitted 
in  an  obviously  insane  condition  to  a  Soldiers'  Home  4^  months 
previously,  and  lived  13  months  subsequent  to  his  admission 
to  the  Hospital.  In  the  Soldiers'  Home  he  was  very  restless, 
constantly  wandered  about,  exhibited  a  defective  memory,  was 
careless  of  his  personal  appearance,  and  imagined  that  strangers 
were  relatives  who  had  been  dead  for  years.  Physically  there 
were  incontinence  of  urine,  chronic  cystitis,  and  marked  tachy- 
cardia. No  family  or  previous  personal  history  could  be  obtained. 
Neurologically,  there  was  tremor  of  the  extended  hands  and 
protruded  tongue,  most  of  the  tendon  reflexes  were  exag- 
gerated, coordination  and  station  were  good,  the  larger  vol- 
untary movements  were  fairly  accurate  and  exact,  but  his  hand- 
writing was  very  shaky.  He  smiled  and  laughed  spasmodically 
and  without  apparent  reason,  and  without  any  apparent 
emotional  accompaniment;  he  responded  very  poorly  to  the 
routine  mental  examination,  forgot  the  questions  which  were 
asked,  and  showed  little  memory  for  his  past  life  and  what 
information  he  gave  was  apparently  incomplete  and  inaccurate; 
he  could  not  find  his  bed;  he  appeared  to  have  no  intelligent 
understanding ;  he  had  no  insignt  into  his  condition ;  no  hallucina- 
tions or  delusions  could  be  elicited;  he  was  disoriented  in  all 
fields,  and  addressed  patients  and  others  by  names  not  theirs, 
apparently  thinking  them  friends  or  relations;  he  was  so  untidy 
that  he  had  to  be  cared  for  in  bed.  Death  was  due  to  hypostatic 
pneumonia.  In  addition,  the  autopsy  showed;  chronic  cystitis, 
nephritis,  hemorrhagic  enteritis,  heart  valves  atheromatous,  and 
circumscribed  atrophy  of  the  frontal  lobes,  but  no  other  cerebral 
lesions.  The  clinical  diagnosis  was  arteriosclerotic  dementia, 
but  the  miscroscopical  examination  of  the  brain  showed  senile 
changes  and  did  not  confirm  the  clinical  diagnosis,  and  the 
case  is,  therefore,  included  in  this  section. 


SYMPTOM ATOLOG1CAL  DIFFERENCES  IN  THE  INSANE      53 

Case  26,  white  male,  had  been  noted  as  insane  for  10  months 
previous  to  his  admission  at  the  age  of  67;  Hospital  residence 
was  8  months.  The  signs  of  insanity  noted  on  the  medical 
certificate  were :  the  patient  wandered  away  from  the  ward,  tore 
his  clothing,  he  showed  mental  confusion,  impairment  of  mem- 
ory, occasional  maniacal  manifestations,  incoherent  talk,  and  was 
sleepless.  The  account  he  gave  of  his  family  and  past  life  was 
incomplete  but  negative.  Physically  he  had  hypertrophied  heart, 
superficial  arteriosclerosis,  slight  arcus  senilis,  his  hearing  was 
impaired,  the  tendon  reflexes  were  mostly  exaggerated,  and  there 
was  a  tremor  of  extended  fingers.  He  was  kept  in  bed  as 
much  as  possible,  and  at  first  was  quiet  and  orderly,  cooperating 
well.  The  mental  examination  showed  a  total  lack  of  orienta- 
tion, no  insight  except  that  at  one  time  the  patient  said  that 
if  the  questions  had  been  asked  a  month  previous  he  would  have 
been  able  to  answer  them;  his  memory  was  very  poor  for  recent 
and  remote  events;  the  only  evidence  of  a  delusion  was  his 
statement  that  he  had  been  robbed  of  thousands  of  dollars;  his 
talk  was  disconnected,  but  his  speech  was  good.  Later  he 
became  restless,  fumbled  with  the  bed  clothing,  was  noisy,  sang 
loudly,  laughed  and  talked  to  himself,  usually  calling  to  horses, 
as  if  he  were  taking  care  of  or  driving  them,  and  advising  those 
around  him  to  get  out  of  the  way  or  the  horses  would  run  over 
them;  he  pulled  the  bed  clothing  and  his  remarks  were  ap- 
parently due  to  ideas  that  he  was  driving ;  he  took  off  his  clothes 
and  went  about  his  room  naked;  apparently  did  not  know  how 
to  put  them  on,  for  he  was  found  putting  his  shirt  over  his 
legs.  The  autopsy  showed  shrinkage  of  the  convolutions  of 
the  frontal  lobes,  but  no  other  gross  cerebral  lesions ;  heart  valves 
atheromatous,  hypostatic  congestion  of  the  lungs,  congestion 
of  the  stomach,  intestines,  liver,  spleen  and  bladder,  and  slight 
fibrosis  of  the  kidneys. 

fc 

Case  27,  white  male,  as  an  inmate  of  a  Soldiers'  Home  was 
considered  to  be  senile  for  nearly  two  years  previous  to  his 
entrance  to  this  Hospital  at  the  age  of  87,  where  he  lived  2 
years  and  5  months. 


54  SHEPHERD  IVORY  FRANZ 

The  medical  certificate  stated  that  he  was  careless  of  his 
personal  appearance,  was  childish,  and,  unless  confined,  wandered 
away  and  became  lost.  Physically  he  was  very  active  for  one 
of  his  age,  and  was  in  good  general  health.  He  showed,  how- 
ever, a  beginning  cataract  in  both  eyes  and  was  very  deaf; 
there  was  sclerosis  of  the  superficial  arteries,  the  muscles  were 
small  (atrophied)  and  their  force  was  slight,  but  movements 
were  accurate  and  quick ;  there  was  a  fine  tremor  of  the  extended 
hands;  gait  was  normal  and  coordination  was  good.  He  was 
quite  cheerful;  he  wandered  about  the  wards  in  an  aimless 
manner;  he  slept  wherever  he  happened  to  sit  down;  he  was 
untidy  in  habits;  he  lost  his  way  on  the  ward;  he  was  com- 
pletely disoriented;  he  seldom  spoke  voluntarily,  but  answered 
cheerfully;  he  was  inclined  to  be  argumentative,  especially  re- 
garding religious  topics;  he  showed  marked  humor;  occasionally 
he  was  excited  and  disagreeable  and  fought  with  other  patients ; 
he  persisted  in  keeping  his  clothes  on  night  and  day ;  his  memory 
was  impaired,  but  he  talked  intelligently  about  historical  events 
of  which  he  had  read;  he  spoke  of  $1,000  which  he  believed  he 
had  and  which  he  wished  to  get  so  that  he  could  go  to  his  friends ; 
this  was  the  only  near-delusion  which  was  elicited.  Death  was 
due  to  cardiovascular  and  renal  diseases ;  the  autopsy  also  showed 
atrophy  of  the  frontal  convolutions,  arteries  sclerosed,  emphy- 
sematous  lungs  and  consolidation  of  the  right  lung,  nutmeg 
liver. 

Case  28,  white  male,  was  noted  to  have  mental  enfeeblement 
4 1/2  months  previous  to  his  admission  at  the  age  of  71 ;  he  lived 
iQl/2  months. 

Neither  family  nor  previous  personal  histories  could  be  ob- 
tained on  account  of  the  patient's  condition.  There  were  no 
previous  attacks  known,  and  the  first  signs  of  mental  enfeeble- 
ment became  evident  only  a  few  months  before  his  entrance  to 
the  Hospital.  Alcoholism  was  given  on  the  medical  certificate 
as  a  probable  cause  of  his  condition,  which  was  noted  to  be  a 
"confusion."  He  believed  himself  to  be  on  board  a  ship,  per- 
secuted, without  his  rights  and  often  asked  when  he  was  to 


SYMPTOMATOLOG1CAL  DIFFERENCES  IN  THE  INSANE      55 

be  murdered;  he  also  had  hallucinations  and  threatened  to  com- 
mit suicide ;  he  was  quiet  and  unassuming.  Neurologically,  hear- 
ing was  much  impaired,  a  slight  degree  of  arcus  senilis  was 
present,  and  the  right  pupil  was  smaller  than  the  left;  there 
were  slight  tremors  of  the  fingers;  skin  sensations  were  some- 
what impaired;  all  tendon  reflexes  were  very  much  diminished 
with  the  exception  of  the  biceps.  He  was  disoriented  for  time, 
place,  and  persons;  his  memory  for  even  the  general  events 
of  his  life  was  very  uncertain;  his  remarks  were  almost  un- 
intelligible; but  he  appeared  to  have  some  insight,  i.e.,  he  said 
he  thought  his  mind  was  affected  in  some  way;  he  took  no 
account  of  the  time  or  condition  of  things,  he  complained  of 
the  weather  being  snowy  and  cold  when  it  was  bright  and 
warm,  and  he  inquired  why  he  was  without  shoes  and  clothes 
with  the  snow  up  to  his  knees ;  he  denied  having  hallucinations ; 
he  also  said  he  did  not  mind  the  snakes,  but  he  was  afraid  of 
the  big  alligators  lying  flat  with  their  eyes  down;  looking  up 
at  the  ceiling  he  talked  at  imaginary  people,  and  his  remarks 
could  not  be  understood,  except  that  he  swore  and  became  very 
excited  and  sang  and  shouted.  At  one  time  he  was  found  to 
have  a  twitching  of  the  eyelids,  and  later  the  left  and  then  the 
right  arm  were  noted  to  be  twitching  (convulsion?)  ;  after  this 
the  Babinski  phenomenon  was  present  on  the  left,  and  he  moved 
the  left  hand  more  than  the  right.  The  autopsy  showed  that  the 
right  hemisphere  was  shorter  than  the  left,  and  there  was  slight 
shrinkage  in  the  left  frontal  region;  no  other  gross  cerebral 
lesions  were  observed;  the  heart  was  dilated,  with  few  athero- 
matous  patches  on  the  valve  leaflets  and  on  the  aorta ;  pulmonary 
tuberculosis  and  hypostatic  congestion;  nephritis.  The  mico- 
scopical  examination  revealed  senile  changes,  although  the 
primary  clinical  diagnosis  had  been  arteriosclerotic  dementia, 
and  the  case  is  therefore  classed  with  the  senile. 

Case  29,  colored  male,  ,his  age  on  admission  was  69,  the 
duration  of  the  mental  disease  was  not  given,  but  the  patient 
had  been  in  this  Hospital  for  more  than  four  years  with  a  mental 
disturbance,  the  nature  of  which  is  not  clear  on  account  of  lack 


56  SHEPHERD  IVORY  FRANZ 

of  clinical  data,  13  years  previously.     He  lived  only  2  months 
subsequent  to  his  second  admission. 

No  family  or  personal  history  of  value  could  be  obtained, 
although  his  previous  residence  in  the  Hospital  is  known.  The 
medical  certificate  stated  that  he  exhibited  delusions  of  wealth, 
was  slovenly  in  his  appearance,  sang  and  danced  for  hours, 
but  at  times  was  irritable  and  quarrelled  with  others,  and  was 
unable  to  repeat  test  phrases.  Syphilis  was  given  as  a  probable 
cause  of  his  condition,  but  a  subsequent  Wassermann  test  was 
negative.  When  received  he  complied  with  all  requests  willingly ; 
he  was  extremely  talkative,  spoke  well  and  intelligently;  told 
how  extensively  he  had  traveled  and  recalled  every  place  and 
event  of  importance,  and  his  memory  seemed  to  be  accurate 
and  exact;  he  spoke  a  few  foreign  phrases  and  thought  he  could 
talk  several  languages;  he  was  bright  and  alert,  was  ready  to 
talk  when  spoken  to  and  ceased  when  requested  and  this  with- 
out apparent  offense;  he  was  restless  and  wished  to  be  doing 
something  all  the  time;  he  denied  having  hallucinations,  and 
his  ideas  of  wealth  were  not  out  of  proportion  to  his  station, 
although  previous  to  his  admission  he  had  peculiar  ideas  regard- 
ing some  financial  dealings  with  others;  he  jumped  from  one 
topic  to  another  in  his  conversation;  he  was  well  oriented  for 
place  and  persons,  but  not  well  for  time;  he  appeared  to  have 
some  insight  into  his  condition  for  he  said  he  was  a  "bit  excited" 
on  his  admission  and  that  his  memory  was  not  good.  He  said 
he  had  fallen  from  the  seventh-story  window  thirty-four  years  pre- 
vious to  his  admission  and  suffered  from  "concussion,"  and  had 
been  bothered  with  this  more  or  less  ever  since.  His  feet  were 
swollen  and  the  skin  over  them  was  glazed,  there  was  marked 
dyspnoea,  radical  arteriosclerosis,  double  arcus  senilis,  great 
emaciation,  pulmonary  tuberculosis  .  The  autopsy  showed  slight 
frontal  shrinkage,  but  no  other  cerebral  lesions,  valvular  heart 
disease,  atheromatous  aorta,  tubercular  lungs,  hypostatic  pneu- 
monia, nephritis.  The  miscroscopical  examination  showed 
marked  senile  changes  in  the  cerebrum,  especially  in  the  frontal 
lobes  although  the  clinical  diagnosis  was  arteriosclerotic  dementia 
(maniacal  excitement). 


SYMPTOMATOLOGY AL  DIFFERENCES  IN  THE  INSANE      57 

Case  30,  white  male,  was  65  at  the  time  of  admission  and  had 
exhibited  mental  disturbances  for  a  year  previous;  he  lived  13 
years  and  2  ^  months. 

The  medical  certificate  stated  that  he  was  admitted  to  a 
Soldiers'  Home  for  various  disabilities,  including  nervous  pros- 
tration, at  the  age  of  54,  and  he  was  placed  in  the  insane  part 
of  the  Home  a  few  months  previous  to  his  admission  to  the 
Hospital,  on  account  of  "delusional  insanity."  It  was  noted  that 
he  was  quiet  and  depressed;  his  memory  was  imperfect  and  his 
perceptions  were  impaired.  On  admission  he  appeared  deeply 
depressed,  he  was  quiet,  and  apparently  he  took  no  interest  in 
his  surroundings;  evidences  of  delusions  or  of  hallucinations 
were  not  found,  but  he  constantly  asked  questions  about  words 
which  he  found  in  the  books  and  papers  he  read,  and  figured 
a  great  deal  on  paper  about  them.  At  times  he  was  cross  and 
disagreeable  to  other  patients  and  fought  them,  and  was  very 
noisy  in  the  halls  and  had  to  be  secluded.  He  appeared  to  have 
peculiar  ideas  about  politics,  taxation,  the  coinage  of  money  and 
the  money  question,  but  it  was  difficult  to  understand  what  he 
meant;  he  drew  maps  of  model  towns  and  sent  them  to  different 
people  to  have  them  adopted ;  he  talked  freely ;  he  was  tidy 
and  cleanly.  Later  he  took  no  interest  in  his  surroundings  and 
the  peculiar  ideas  noted  above  became  exaggerated  and  further 
elaborated;  he  also  thought  that  the  patients  carried  electricity 
around  with  them  and  shot  it  into  him  so  that  he  was  prevented 
from  urinating,  or  that  they  hypnotized  him  and  played  witch- 
craft upon  him;  he  was  described  at  different  times  as  being 
"completely  disoriented"  and  "completely  oriented,"  and  as  hav- 
ing a  "good  memory"  and  a  "poor  memory" ;  he  stated  that  at 
night  when  he  was  alone  and  in  bed  he  used  to  hear  the  voices 
of  his  enemies  speaking  on  political  matters;  he  wrote  inco- 
herently and  almost  incessantly.  His  gait  was  slow,  there  were 
no  paralyses,  the  radial  arteries  were  sclerosed,  general  tremors 
were  present.  Death  was  due  to  chronic  bronchitis.  The  autopsy 
also  showed :  atrophy  of  the  anterior  cerebral  lobes ;  atheromatous 
aortic  valves,  consolidation  of  the  right  lower  lung;  chronic 
cholecystitis;  chronic  inflammation  of  the  liver,  spleen,  kidneys 


58  SHEPHERD  IVORY  FRANZ 

and  bladder.  Three  clinical  diagnoses  were  made  at  different 
times:  (i)  undifferentiated  psychosis  (dementia);  (2)  paranoid 
state  associated  with  arteriosclerosis;  and  (3)  senile  depression. 

Case  31,  white  female;  77  years  old  at  the  time  of  admission; 
she  lived  8  years  and  3  months  subsequently;  the  duration 
of  the  mental  disease  at  the  time  of  admission  was  not  learned. 

No  family  or  previous  personal  history,  and  no  physical,  in- 
cluding neurological,  examination  accompany  the  records,  al- 
though on  admission  it  was  noted  that  her  health  and  condition 
were  good  for  one  of  her  age.  She  was  much  confused  and 
wandered  around  the  ward  in  an  aimless  manner;  she  was  free 
from  delusions  and  hallucinations;  she  was  forgetful  and  easily 
irritated,  and  childish  and  dependent  in  manner;  she  was  tidy 
in  habits.  A  year  after  her  admission  she  had  an  epileptiform 
convulsion,  which  left  her  very  much  confused  and  reduced 
mentally  but  did  not  result  in  any  paralysis.  Epileptiform  con- 
vulsions were  present  at  irregular  intervals,  and  previous  and 
subsequent  to  these  she  became  confused.  Three  years  after 
her  admission  to  the  Hospital  she  was  noted  to  have  many 
delusions,  especially  those  of  great  wealth,  that  the  attending 
physician  was  her  son,  that  she  was  going  to  will  him  a  great 
deal  of  property;  she  often  said  she  had  just  been  visited  by 
her  relatives;  she  thought  at  times  she  was  Queen  Victoria, 
and  at  other  times  that  the  Queen  was  her  best  friend.  She 
was  noted  to  be  picking  imaginary  objects  from  the  floor  and 
said  she  was  gathering  money  there.  She  became  hypochondria- 
cal;  her  memory  was  defective;  she  lacked  insight;  there  was 
poverty  of  ideas;  she  was  disoriented;  and  her  reasoning  and 
judgment  were  impaired.  For  fifteen  months  before  death  she 
remained  in  bed  helpless,  totally  indifferent  to  her  surroundings ; 
she  never  initiated  conversation,  and  answered  only  in  mono- 
syllables. The  autopsy  showed  that  the  convolutions  of  the 
anterior  lobes  were  markedly  shrunken;  in  addition  there  were 
sclerosis  of  the  aorta,  mitral  and  aortic  valvular  lesions,  purulent 
bronchitis,  interstitial  nephritis  and  chronic  cystitis. 


SYMPTOM ATOLOGICAL  DIFFERENCES  IN  THE  INSANE      59 

Case  32,  white  male,  admitted  at  the  age  of  74.  The  duration 
of  his  mental  disturbance  previous  to  entrance  to  the  Hospital 
was  not  recorded.  He  died  in  6  months. 

This  patient  was  admitted  to  a  Soldiers'  Home  where  he 
was  found  to  have  emphysema,  spinal  curvature  and  an  old 
fracture  of  the  ribs  on  the  left  side.  While  in  that  institution 
he  had  a  slight  fever  and  delirium  which  were  thought  to  be 
due  to  gastritis,  and  following  this  he  was  noted  to  show  evi- 
dence of  dementia.  He  talked  incoherently ;  he  wandered  around 
the  ward  and  tried  to  get  out;  he  was  unable  to  find  his  bed, 
and  constantly  disturbed  other  patients  by  getting  into  their 
beds;  he  had  "no  conception  of  anything."  Physically,  he  was 
poorly  nourished,  the  mitral  and  aortic  valves  of  the  heart  were 
slightly  involved,  there  was  sclerosis  of  the  superficial  vessels, 
there  was  bilateral  arcus  senilis;  the  pupils  were  unequal  and 
irregular,  the  right  gave  the  Argyll-Robertson  reaction;  there 
was  tremor  of  the  fingers;  the  tendon  reflexes  were  not  elicited; 
the  Wassermann  reaction  with  the  blood  serum  was  negative. 
He  appeared  to  have  no  insight  into  his  condition,  but  at  one 
time  he  remarked  there  were  things  in  his  mind  he  knew  were 
"not  right";  emotionally  he  was  indifferent;  he  was  disoriented 
for  time,  place  and  persons;  his  memory  was  greatly  impaired; 
he  was  unable  to  find  his  seat  in  the  dining  room  or  his  bed 
at  night;  he  was  up  and  dressed  and  was  fairly  tidy;  as  a  rule 
he  was  quiet  and  orderly,  but  later  became  very  much  confused, 
restless  and  untidy  in  habits.  A  right  lobar  pneumonia  developed, 
from  which  the  patient  died;  at  the  autopsy  there  were  also 
found :  cerebral  convolutions  atrophied  anteriorly ;  calcification 
of  the  aortic  and  mitral  valves ;  atheromatous  arch  of  the  aorta  ; 
tubercular  scars  in  the  left  lung;  liver  and  spleen  somewhat 
cirrhotic;  cortices  of  kidneys  markedly  thinned. 

Case  33,  white  male,  admitted  at  the  age  of  79,  had  been  ob- 
served for  over  a  year  to  show  signs  of  mental  impairment,  and 
lived  for  7^  years  after  admission. 

The  medical  certificate  stated  that  he  had  an  uncontrollable 
temper,  had  hysterical  attacks,  delusions  of  persecution  and  loss 


60  SHEPHERD  IVORY  FRANZ 

of  memory.  Physically  there  were:  slight  peripheral  arterios- 
clerosis ;  hearing  was  very  much  diminished ;  the  voluntary  move- 
ments were  normal  in  accuracy  and  rapidity,  but  they  lacked 
force;  the  knee  jerks  were  absent.  The  family  and  previous 
personal  history  was  poor  because  the  patient's  memory  for  re- 
cent and  remote  events  was  defective  and  what  was  obtained 
showed  nothing  of  interest  or  importance.  The  patient's  expres- 
sion was  stupid,  he  was  rather  untidy,  he  appeared  to  be  simple- 
minded  and  rather  childish ;  usually  he  was  good  natured,  happy 
and  contented,  but  at  times  he  was  irritable  and  abusive ;  he  was 
very  industrious  making  baskets  which  he  tried  to  sell ;  he  talked 
and  associated  with  the  other  patients  in  a  normal  manner;  his 
memory  for  recent  and  remote  events  was  not  good,  but,  on  the 
other  hand,  was  not  entirely  absent;  he  conversed  coherently, 
relevantly  and  freely ;  he  was  oriented,  showed  fairly  good  judg- 
ment, but  exhibited  no  insight.  Fluid  accumulated  in  the  ab- 
domen and  for  this  he  was  tapped  twice,  but  died.  At  the  autopsy 
the  chief  findings  were:  fifteen  liters  of  fluid  in  the  abdominal 
cavity;  marked  sclerosis  of  the  abdominal  aorta;  peritonitis; 
sclerosis  of  the  liver  and  kidney;  and  marked  shrinkage  of  the 
anterior  cerebral  convolutions. 

Case  34,  white  male,  exhibited  mental  changes  for  nearly  3 
years  previous  to  his  admission  to  the  Hospital  at  the  age  of  80 ; 
and  lived  I  year  2^/2  months  subsequently. 

The  patient's  family  history  and  the  account  of  his  previous 
life  were  not  well  obtained,  but  what  was  learned  was  unimpor- 
tant. The  medical  certificate  reported  him  to  be  childish,  restless, 
inclined  to  wander  away  and  become  lost.  His  general  attitude 
was  one  of  weakness,  or  weariness  and  fatigue;  his  gait  was 
shuffling,  and  slow;  he  had  impaired  vision  (beginning  cataract)  ; 
and  defective  speech.  Neurologically  he  showed:  arcus  senilis; 
unequal  pupils,  the  left  failed  to  react  to  light;  the  knee  jerks 
were  diminished;  there  was  a  slight  ankle  clonus;  there  were 
tremors  of  the  tongue,  lips  and  face ;  a  slight  Romberg  was  pres- 
ent. He  was  noted  to  be  harmless,  agreeable,  and  cooperative; 


SYMPTOM ATOLOGICAL  DIFFERENCES  IN  THE  INSANE      61 

he  was  absolutely  disoriented;  his  memory  was  very  poor  both 
for  recent  and  remote  events;  he  was  somewhat  confused  and 
was  easily  upset;  emotionally  he  was  unstable,  and  occasionally 
irritable,  and  possibly  on  the  borderline  of  depression;  he  had 
nothing  to  say  voluntarily;  he  preferred  to  be  let  alone  and  he 
sat  in  one  corner  and  did  not  associate  with  the  other  patients ;  he 
appeared  to  have  fair  insight  into  his  condition ;  his  general  com- 
prehension, reasoning  and  judgment  did  not  seem  impaired  when 
his  mind  was  occupied  with  simple  things;  his  mental  associa- 
tions were  not  very  active;  his  ideation  was  limited  in  amount; 
he  paid  little  or  no  attention  to  his  environment.  He  became 
untidy,  refused  to  answer  questions  and  used  profane,  vulgar  and 
abusive  language  to  his  questioners.  The  autopsy  showed :  slight 
atrophy  of  the  anterior  cerebral  convolutions,  but  no  other  gross 
cerebral  lesions ;  aorta  and  aortic  valves  atheromatous ;  bronchitis ; 
pulmonary  tubercular  nobules;  pulmonary  emphysema;  cirrhosis 
of  the  liver ;  fibrous  spleen  and  kidney ;  the  right  adrenal  contained 
an  overgrowth  and  an  hemorrhagic  area. 

Case  35,  white  male,  has  been  mentally  changed  for  a  year  pre- 
vious to  admission,  at  the  age  of  61.  Hospital  residence  was  n 
years  and  2  months. 

This  patient  was  sent  to  the  Hospital  on  account  of  de- 
lusions of  "strange  and  impossible  happenings"  and  hallucina- 
tions of  hearing  (fictitious  voices)  and  of  vision  (mysterious 
objects).  Physically  he  showed  a  dilated  heart,  bleeding 
hemorrhoids,  and  a  right  inguinal  hernia.  Neurologically  his 
movements  were  deliberate,  slow  and  rather  weak;  his  coordina- 
tion seemed  somewhat  impaired,  although  his  gait  was  firm  and 
steady;  a  slight  fibrillary  tremor  of  the  tongue  was  observed; 
hearing  was  defective.  He  appeared  to  comprehend  what  was 
said  to  him,  but  was  deliberate  in  answering;  his  memory  was 
good,  somewhat  'better  for  remote  than  for  recent  events;  his 
reasoning  and  judgment  appeared  to  be  below  par;  he  heard 
voices  outside  his  door  at  night;  he  was  mildly  depressed  but 
claimed  to  be  fairly  well  satisfied  with  his  surroundings  (ex- 


62  SHEPHERD  IVORY  FRANZ 

cept  for  his  delusions) ;  he  also  showed  some  irritability. 
Later  he  became  more  irritable,  and  at  times  he  was  ugly 
and  insulting;  he  refused  to  answer  questions;  he  was  dis- 
trustful and  suspicious,  and  exhibited  delusions  of  perse- 
cution (he  claimed  that  the  physicians  had  him  "wired,"  that 
they  conspired  to  keep  him  in  the  Hospital,  and  hold  him  for 
robbery  and  mistreatment;  and  he  refused  to  converse  with 
them)  ;  he  claimed  that  the  patients  ought  to  be  sent  to  school  and 
educated ;  he  was  oriented ;  he  was  tidy ;  he  was  usually  quiet  and 
orderly,  but  occasionally  he  became  indignant,  irritable  and 
wrought  up  over  his  detention.  Delusions  of  grandeur  were 
added  to  those  of  persecution  (he  thought  himself  to  be  a  very 
rich  man  and  that  the  Government  was  robbing  him  of  thousands 
of  dollars  daily)  ;  his  memory  became  poor;  he  was  disoriented 
for  time;  when  talking  with  the  physicians,  regarding  whom  he 
had  delusions,  he  became  so  emotional  that  his  voice  trembled ;  he 
refused  to  answer  questions  regarding  his  condition;  he  was 
usually  quiet,  orderly  and  well-behaved  except  when  approached 
by  one  regarding  whom  he  had  delusions;  later  he  believed  the 
attendants  were  putting  poison  into  his  food  and  drinks,  trying 
to  shave  him  against  his  will  and  injure  him  in  other  ways ;  insight 
into  his  condition  was  lacking.  Death  was  due  to  cardiovascular 
and  renal  diseases;  the  autopsy  also  showed  shrinkage  of  the 
cerebral  convolutions  over  the  anterior  two-thirds  and  athero- 
matous  cerebral  vessels. 

Case  36,  colored  female ;  the  duration  of  the  mental  disease  at 
the  time  oi  admission  was  not  stated  in  the  medical  certificate; 
she  lived  in  the  Hospital  7  years  and  5  months. 

The  age  of  this  patient  was  unknown,  but  she  was  undoubtedly 
old  at  the  time  she  was  admitted  from  the  Alms  House ;  there  she 
had  been  observed  to  be  "maniacal,"  i.e.,  very  much  excited ;  she 
could  not  answer  questions  intelligently  and  at  times  showed  that 
she  had  delusions  that  people  were  after  her;  she  was  noisy, 
especially  at  night.  Her  facial  expression  was  one  of  apathy; 
her  gait  was  slow  and  feeble;  the  physical  examination  showed 
nothing  abnormal;  neurologically  she  showed  fine  tremors  of 


the  extremities,  sight  and  hearing  were  defective.  She  was 
somewhat  disoriented  and  there  was  some  clouding  of  con- 
sciousness; she  did  not  appear  to  appreciate  her  surroundings; 
her  memory  for  remote  events  was  fair,  but  poor  for  recent 
events;  in  talking  she  lapsed  into  incoherency,  goal  ideas  were 
lost;  her  reasoning  and  judgment  were  impaired;  she  thought 
she  could  talk  with  the  Lord,  and  she  became  religiously  excited 
at  times,  but  as  a  rule  she  sat  quietly  in  the  ward,  taking  no  in- 
terest; she  was  tidy  in  habits.  She  exhibited  the  signs  of  gradual 
mental  enfeeblement;  memory  became  practically  a  blank,  and  she 
became  untidy  in  habits;  she  did  not  want  to  be  interfered  with 
by  the  nurses,  and  occasionally  she  showed  a  rather  cross  and 
irritable  disposition;  she  sat  in  one  chair  with  an  apron  over  her 
head  for  hours;  she  never  spoke  unless  spoken  to  and  then  al- 
ways complained  of  being  burned  up  by  fire;  the  reason  for  cover- 
ing her  head  could  not  be  learned.  Death  was  due  to  pulmonary 
hypostasis  and  hemorrhagic  cystitic;  the  autopsy  showed  in 
addition  generalized  arteriosclerosis,  cardiac  atrophy,  chronic 
diffuse  nephritis  and  atrophy  of  the  anterior  two-thirds  of  the 
cerebral  convolutions. 

Case  37,  white  male,  had  been  mentally  deranged  for  2  years 
previous  to  his  admission  at  the  age  of  74 ;  he  lived  in  the  Hospi- 
tal 3  years  and  2  months. 

The  family  history  was  negative.  At  the  age  of  43,  as  a 
veteran  of  the  Civil  War,  he  was  admitted  to  a  Soldiers'  Home,  for 
"physical  disability  and  mental  incapacity."  The  physical  dis- 
ability was  a  contusion  erf  the  right  shoulder;  the  character  of 
the  mental  incapacity  was  not  noted,  but  could  not  have  been  a 
marked  mental  change  since  the  patient  was  able  to  take  care  of 
himself  and  was  permitted  to  go  at  will.  Twenty-four  years 
later,  at  the  age  of  67,  he  was  noted  to  have  "impaired  cerebra- 
tion," but  this  term  is  not  defined ;  six  years  subsequently  he  was 
noted  to  be  senile;  to  have  impaired  cerebration  and  delusions  of 
persecution  (he  thought  he  was  to  be  hanged  for  misdemeanors 
of  which  he  was  not  guilty)  ;  he  was  inclined  to  be  talkative,  but 
his  conversation  was  disconnected ;  there  was  inability  to  concen- 


64  SHEPHERD  IVORY  FRANZ 

trate  his  thoughts;  he  showed  impaired  judgment  and  reasoning. 
On  one  occasion  a  rope,  carefully  and  securely  fixed,  was  found 
in  his  locker,  although  it  is  not  known  that  he  had  attempted  or 
planned  suicide.  Physically  he  was  quite  active  for  his  age,  and 
his  physical  condition  good  with  the  exception  of  difficulty  of  hear- 
ing and  right  inguinal  hernia.  On  admission  he  was  restless  and 
confused ;  he  showed  no  interest  in  his  surroundings ;  he  was  un- 
tidy in  appearance,  but  tidy  in  habits ;  it  was  difficult  to  make  him 
understand,  and  he  'failed  to  answer  questions ;  no  delusions  could 
be  elicited,  although  he  appeared  to  be  apprehensive  and  afraid 
that  any  one  who  approached  him  would  do  him  harm ;  his  con- 
sciousness was  clouded;  he  was  disoriented  for  time  and  place; 
his  memory  and  intellect  were  very  much  impaired ;  his  emotional 
tone  was  one  of  indifference.  Soon  he  became  untidy  in  habits, 
and 'there  remained  to  him  only  a  remnant  of  his  former  knowl- 
edge ;  he  was  kept  in  'bed ;  he  took  no  interest  in  his  surroundings ; 
when  approached  and  questioned  he  cried  and  whined  pitifully, 
he  volunteered  no  information  and  he  answered  few  questions; 
he  was  entirely  disoriented;  he  seemed  to  remember  the  events 
of  his  childhood  but  none  of  recent  date;  no  delusions  or  hallu- 
cinations could  be  elicited.  He  was  restless  and  pulled  and  re- 
arranged his  bed  clothing  frequently  and  resisted  attempts  to 
help  him.  Death  was  due  to  bronchopneumonia.  The  autopsy 
showed  marked  shrinkage  of  the  f  rontoparietal  convolutions. 

Case  38,  white  male,  was  admitted  to  the  Hospital  at  the  age  of 
72 ;  his  mental  disturbance  began  2  months  previous  to,  and 
he  lived  only  i  month  after  his  admission. 

For  two  months  previous  to  admission  the  patient  was  childish, 
forgetful,  disoriental,  and  had  a  depression.  His  family  history, 
as  far  as  it  could  be  ascertained,  was  negative.  Physically  he 
showed  slightly  enlarged  heart,  double  arcus  senilis,  and  defec- 
tive hearing;  Wassermann  reaction  with  the  blood  serum  was 
negative;  albumin  and  casts  were  found  in  the  urine.  Neurolo- 
gically  he  showed :  sluggish  accommodation  reactions,  and  irregu- 
lar pupils;  deep  reflexes  diminished;  slight  Romberg  sign;  gait 
was  very  feeble;  muscles  wasted  and  atrophic;  voluntary  move- 


SYMPTOMATOLOGICAL  DIFFERENCES  IN  THE  INSANE      65 

ments  feeble ;  irregular  tremors  of  the  fingers.  Mentally  he  said 
he  was  sad,  but  usually  he  appeared  to  be  indifferent;  he  had  a 
poor  memory ;  he  was  not  oriented  .for  persons,  and  only  partially 
for  time  and  place ;  insight  was  lacking ;  no  hallucinations  or  delu- 
sions were  elicited;  his  speech  was  good.  The  patient  developed 
diarrhoea  and  died  from  exhaustion;  the  autopsy  revealed  no 
arteriosclerosis ;  the  cerebral  convolutions  were  shrunken  over  the 
frontal  lobes  and  the  posterior  portions  of  the  parietal  lobes,  but 
section  of  the  brain  showed  no  other  changes;  aortic  atheroma; 
pulmonary  tuberculosis;  and  parenchymatous  nephritis. 

SENILE  DEMENTIA,  DISCUSSION 

These  fourteen  cases  are  not  entirely  alike  as  far  as  lesions  are 
concerned,  and,  it  will  be  noted,  some  diagnostic  questions  have 
been  raised  regarding  some  of  them.  Case  28  has  been  noted 
as  being  a  possible  alcoholic  psychosis,  and  some  of  the  hallucina- 
tions which  this  patient  had  are  suggestive  of  the  alcoholic  de- 
lirium. Patients  29,  30,  and  37  were  also  considered  at  one  time 
to  be  rather  doubtful.  Patient  29  was  admitted  to  the  Hospital 
for  the  first  time  at  the  age  of  56,  was  subsequently  discharged, 
but  readmitted  at  the  age  of  69.  Patient  30,  on  the  other  hand, 
had  been  noted  to  have  had  "nervous  prostration"  eleven  years 
previous  to  his  admission  to  the  Hospital,  but  the  symptoms  which 
he  exhibited  at  that  time  were  not  recorded,  and  it  is  impossible 
to  determine  the  nature  of  the  condition.  Since  the  term  "nervous 
prostration"  is  so  inaccurately  used  by  general  practitioners,  it  is 
difficult  to  be  certain  that  the  patient  exhibited  anything  more 
than  a  disinclination  for  mental  and  physical  work.  Patient  27 
also  had  been  noted  as  exhibiting  "mental  incapacity"  thirty-one 
years  previous  to  his  admission  to  the  Hospital,  but,  as  has  been 
noted  in  the  case  history,  this  was  probably  a  very  general  term, 
and  it  may  have  been  only  a  means  of  having  him  enrolled  as  an 
inmate  of  the  Soldiers'  Home  in  which  he  spent  these  years.  Pa- 
tient 31  had  epileptiform  convulsions,  but  no  previous  history  of 
disturbances  of  this  nature  was  obtained,  and  it  is  likely  that 
these  convulsive  seizures  were  due  to  the  degenerative  cerebral 
conditions  which  mentally  resulted  in  the  dementia. 


66  SHEPHERD  IVORY  FRANZ 

Regarding  the  cerebral  conditions,  similar  variations  are  noted. 
Patient  28  exhibited  what  appeared  to  be  an  unilateral  atrophy  on 
the  left  side  of  the  cerebrum.  The  description  of  the  atrophy  in 
case  37  is  probably  to  be  taken  to  be  the  equivalent  of  that  which 
has  been  noted  in  cases  35  and  36,  namely,  that  it  covered  the 
frontal,  central  and  parietal  regions  of  the  brain,  in  this  way 
taking  in  approximately  two-thirds  of  the  convexity.  The  brain 
of  patient  38,  like  that  of  patient  28,  also  showed  rather  circum- 
scribed atrophies,  and  it  has  been  noted  that  these  were  in  the 
posterior  parietal  lobes  as  well  as  in  the  frontal  lobes.  This  case 
is  added  for  the  purpose  of  comparison  with  those  cases  which 
are  strictly  'frontal  atrophies,  and  for  the  purpose  of  comparison 
with  the  extensive  lesions  which  were  found  in  cases  35,  36,  and 
37.  The  degree  of  atrophy  in  these  cases  was  varied,  cases  28,  29, 
and  34  exhibiting  only  a  slight  amount  of  atrophy  while  cases  31 
and  33  showed  a  marked  degree  of  shrinkage. 

In  general  the  mentality  of  these  patients  did  not  greatly  differ. 
All  showed  a  considerable  degree  of  dementia.  They  had  little 
intelligence  or  understanding  of  what  went  on  about  them;  they 
were  at  times  unable  to  answer  questions ;  they  had  become  child- 
ish, slovenly  in  appearance,  and  they  wandered  around  the  wards 
aimlessly.  At  the  same  time,  they  were  mostly  harmless  and 
usually  quiet,  agreeable,  and  sometimes  apparently  stupid,  but  at 
times  they  became  restless  and  irritated.  Patient  34,  who  ex- 
hibited only  a  slight  degree  of  atrophy  of  the  frontal  regions,  was 
apparently  as  demented  as  any  of  the  other  cases,  and  patients 
28  and  29,  whose  brains  were  also  noted  to  exhibited  only  slight 
atrophies,  were  at  the  same  time  considerably  demented.  On  the 
other  hand,  the  marked  degree  of  cerebral  atrophy  which  was 
found  in  the  brains  of  patients  31  and  33  did  not  appear  to 
bring  about  any  greater  degree  of  dementia  or  loss  of  mentality 
than  in  the  other  cases.  These  two  patients  were  confused  or 
wandered  away,  or  were  stupid,  simple-minded  or  childish,  but 
in  these  respects  they  were  not  any  less  mentally  active  or  men- 
tally endowed  than  patient  25,  who  was  noted  to  exhibit  "no 
intelligent  understanding,"  or  than  patient  26,  who  was  quiet, 
confused,  or  than  patient  27,  who  was  childish  and  wandered 


SYMPTOMATOLOGICAL  DIFFERENCES  IN  THE  INSANE      67 

away.  The  relation  of  the  general  mental  condition  oi  dementia 
to  the  frontal,  or  to  the  distributed  anterior,  or  to  the  even  more 
extensive  frontal-parietal  atrophies  is  not  a  definite  one,  and  it  is 
apparent  that  in  these  cases,  as  well  as  in  those  which  have  pre- 
viously been  considered,  the  relation  of  the  degree  of  atrophy  to 
the  degree  of  dementia  is  not  simple. 

Most  oi  these  patients  exhibited  rather  marked  degrees  of 
motor  disorder.  Tremors  of  the  hands,  tongue,  face,  or  fingers 
were  found  in  nine  of  these  patients,  and  it  is  of  interest  to  note 
that  with  the  exception  of  patient  29  the  frontal  cases  showed 
these  disturbances  as  much  as  those  cases  in  which  the  atrophies 
extended  beyond  the  limits  of  the  frontal  lobes.  In  fact,  those 
patients  whose  brains  were  found  to  have  the  more  extensive 
atrophies  (patients  31,  33  and  37)  did  not  show  motor  distur- 
bances of  this  character.  Patients  27,  29,  33,  and  37  were  active 
for  individuals  of  their  age,  and  although  in  connection  with 
general  muscular  atrophy  they  usually  showed  a  slight  amount  of 
force  in  their  movements,  their  movements  were  accurate  and 
quick.  It  will  be  seen  that  some  of  these  patients  were  those  in 
which  the  precentral  region  was  also  included  in  the  atrophic 
zone.  In  these  cases,  aside  from  the  tremors,  the  five  patients 
with  frontal  atrophies  (including  case  38)  were  apparently  motor- 
ially  more  capable  than  those  patients  with  the  more  extensive 
atrophies.  This,  however,  is  only  true  in  a  general  way.  The 
cases  showed  such  extensive  divergences  in  this  particaular  that 
they  can  not  be  considered  to  be  correlated  with  any  special  de- 
gree or  with  any  special  extent  of  the  pathological  conditions  oi 
cerebral  atrophy. 

The  conversation  of  most  of  these  patients  was  disconnected 
and  frequently  incoherent.  Their  remarks  were  often  almost 
unintelligible.  They  answered  questions  in  an  irrelevant  manner, 
sometimes  slowly  or,  as  it  has  been  described,  deliberately,  and  at 
times  they  refused  to  answer  or  failed  to  answer  at  all.  These 
variations  from  the  normal  activity  were  found  in  practically  all 
these  fourteen  cases,  although  there  is  one  exception,  viz.,  patient 
38,  who  not  only  answered  properly  and  intelligently,  but  his 
speech,  i.e.,  his  enunciation  was  good.  In  the  case  of  speech  as 


68  SHEPHERD  IVORY  FRANZ 

distinguished  from  conversation,  it  was  'found  that  many  of  these 
cases  exhibited  no  specific  alterations,  although  patient  29  has 
been  described  as  having  been  unable  to  repeat  test  phrases,  and 
patient  34  was  noted  to  exhibit  defective  speech. 

The  emotional  tone  of  patients  27  and  28  is  not  specifically 
mentioned,  and  conclusions  regarding  their  conditions  in  this  re- 
spect must  be  drawn  from  the  other  facts  in  the  case  histories. 
We  may  conclude  from  the  general  accounts  that  patient  27 
was  usually  cheerful,  but  at  times,  on  account  of  his  delusions, 
apparently  depressed,  and,  as  has  been  noted  in  the  account  given 
above,  he  was  occasionally  excited,  disagreeable  and  pugnacious. 
The  hallucinations  which  were  evident  in  patient  28  and  the  delu- 
sions which  he  gave  voice  to  did  not  appear  to  bring  about  very 
marked  emotional  reactions,  although  he  was  excited  at  times. 
As  a  rule,  the  other  patients  were  usually  indifferent;  they  were 
quiet ;  some  of  them  were  stupid,  childish,  and  as  may  be  concluded 
from  their  general  mental  symptoms,  they  ranged  in  their  affec- 
tive states  from  indifference  to  excitability,  or  to  feelings  of  well- 
being,  or  to  a  depression  or  sadness.  Patient  25  was  perhaps  the 
only  one  who  exhibited  no  evident  emotional  reaction,  although 
it  should  be  remarked  that  he  laughed  and  smiled  spasmodically. 
Externally  he  appeared  to  be  emotionally  labile,  but  in  reality  he 
had  no  apparent  affective  concomitant  with  these  reactions.  The 
relation  of  these  emotional  conditions  to  the  extents  and  degrees 
of  atrophy  is  not  apparent.  Those  patients  with  frontal  lesions 
appeared  to  be  as  much  affected  as  those  with  the  more  extensive 
lesions,  and  the  condition  of  marked  atrophy  in  patients  31  and 
33,  it  will  be  noted,  did  not  give  rise  to  any  special  degree  or 
character  of  affective  phenomena. 

Eight  oi  these  patients  showed  at  no  time  any  evidence  of  hallu- 
cinations. The  other  six  patients  did  have  hallucinations,  with 
a  possible  exception  of  patient  26.  The  latter  patient,  as  the 
case  history  shows,  laughed  and  talked  to  himself  and  had  delu- 
sions of  driving  horses  or  of  calling  to  them,  and,  like  patient  22, 
he  was  found  calling  to  horses  as  if  he  were  taking  care  o'f  or 
driving  them.  Whether  these  symptoms  should  be  interpreted  to 
be  entirely  delusionary  or  to  be  partially  hallucinatory,  as  if  he 


SYMPTOMATOLOGICAL  DIFFERENCES  IN  THE  INSANE      69 

had  the  particular  sensations  in  the  hands  and  arms  of  driving  or 
of  rubbing  down  the  horses,  or  of  the  visual  experiences  of  seeing 
the  horses,  cannot  be  determined.  It  appears  probable,  however, 
that  the  delusion  of  his  taking  care  of  and  driving  these  animals 
was  due  to  the  presence  of  hallucinations  as  much  as  to  anything 
else.  The  hallucinations  of  patient  28  are  much  like  those  of  the 
alcoholic  delirium,  as  has  been  noted  above.  The  hallucinations 
of  alligators  and  snakes  were  especially  like  those  which  a  patient 
with  delirium  tremens  experiences,  and  the  evident  disorientation 
regarding  the  seasons  when  he  complained  of  the  cold  weather, 
with  snow  on  the  ground,  when  in  reality  it  was  bright  and  warm, 
and  when  he  complained  of  his  being  without  shoes  and  clothing 
with  the  snow  up  to  his  knees,  appeared  to  be  much  more  like  the 
paresthesias  which  an  alcoholic  might  have.  This  apparent  delu- 
sion, or  disorientation  for  time,  appeared,  therefore,  to  be  much 
more  like  a  tactual  paresthesia,  or  to  be  dependent  upon  such  a 
condition,  although  it  is  not  certain  that  we  can  exclude  the  visual 
element.  In  the  expression  df  her  delusions  patient  36  also  gave 
evidence  that  she  had  paresthesias,  for  it  will  be  remem<bered  that 
she  complained  of  being  burned  up  by  fire.  It  is  also  possible  that 
her  delusion  regarding  her  ability  to  talk  with  the  Lord  may  have 
had  as  a  basis  the  presence  of  auditory  hallucinations  of  voices. 
Patients  30  and  31  at  the  time  of  their  entrance  to  the  Hospital 
were  noted  to  be  free  'from  hallucinations,  but  at  a  later  date  both 
of  these  patients  gave  evidence  of  the  presence  of  these  abnormal- 
ities. Patient  30  complained  that  he  heard  voices  at  night.  At  the 
same  time  it  should  be  remembered  that  he  also  complained  that 
electricity  had  been  used  upon  him,  which  had  prevented  his 
urination.  Patient  31  was  found  trying  to  pick  imaginary  objects 
from  the  floor.  Whether  or  not  this  reaction  was  due  to  a  visual 
hallucination  or  to  a  combination  of  visual  and  tactile  hallucina- 
tions was  not  determined.  The  delusion  of  patient  30  regarding 
the  action  of  electricity  was  undoubtedly  of  an  hallucinatory  na- 
ture, as  has  been  mentioned,  the  particular  hallucination  being  of 
the  organic  type.  It  is  of  interest  in  this  connection  to  note  that 
these  patients  who  exhibited  hallucinations  showed  these  abnor- 
malities more  frequently  in  the  field  of  the  skin  sensations  than 


7o  SHEPHERD  IVORY  FRANZ 

in  other  fields.  From  what  we  know  regarding  the  functions  of 
the  postcentral  areas  of  the  brain  it  might  be  expected  that  this 
kind  of  hallucination  would  be  more  common  in  those  patients 
who  exhibited  atrophy  in  the  parietal  region,  viz.,  patients  35, 
36,  and  37,  but  of  these  three  patients  only  one  had  hallucinations 
oi  this  character.  This  woman  complained  of  being  burned  up 
by  fire.  Patients  26  and  28,  whose  brains  were  only  atrophied 
in  the  frontal  lobes,  also  had  the  same  kind  of  delusions,  appar- 
ently based  upon  tactile  or  organic  hallucinations.  From  these 
facts  it  appears  that  the  hallucinations  in  these  cases  are  not  di- 
rectly connected  with  the  degree  or  the  extent  of  the  atrophies 
which  have  been  recorded. 

Three  of  the  patients  were  entirely  free  'from  delusions  of  any 
kind.  Two  others,  patients  25  and  37,  were  somewhat  doubtful, 
although  it  appears  that  patient  25  in  mistaking  individuals  and 
calling  them  by  names  not  their  own  was  suffering  from  a  defect 
of  memory  or  otf  orientation  for  persons,  and  did  not  have,  pri- 
marily, a  delusion.  It  is  impossible  in  the  case  of  patient  37  to 
determine  whether  the  ideas  of  persecution  which  he  spoke 
of  were  or  were  not  delusions.  At  any  rate,  they  were  not  par- 
ticularly evident,  although  it  should  be  remembered  that  he 
frequently  appeared  to  be  apprehensive,  as  if  some  harm  would 
happen  to  him  or  as  i'f  he  were  being  persecuted.  Delusions  of 
persecution  were  also  found  in  patients  26,  28,  30,  33,  35,  and  36. 
These  gave  way  to  delusions  of  grandeur  or  of  exaltation  in  the 
case  of  patient  35,  and  perhaps  also  in  the  case  of  patient  36. 
Delusions  of  grandeur  were  also  given  expression  by  patients  27, 
29,  and  31.  Although  patients  25,  30,  and  31  did  not  show  any 
evidence  of  the  presence  of  delusions  at  the  time  of  their  admis- 
sion to  the  institution,  at  later  dates  delusions,  of  which  mention 
has  been  made  in  the  individual  case  histories,  were  in  evidence. 
The  association  of  delusions  with  lesions  of  the  frontal  region, 
in  view  of  the  lack  of  these  abnormalities  in  cases  32,  34,  and  38, 
and  possibly  also  37  and  25,  can  not  be  said  to  be  demonstrated 
by  these  series  of  cases.  If  we  believe  that  the  "Personenverken- 
nung"  of  patient  25  to  be  a  real  delusion,  we  can  conclude  that  all 
of  the  simple  frontal  cases  exhibited  delusions.  Opposed  to  this, 


however,  is  the  fact  that  the  extension  of  the  atrophies  beyond 
the  frontal  region  in  the  other  nine  cases  did  not  always  result  in 
delusions.  Case  38  is  particularly  interesting  in  this  connection 
because  the  frontal  region  was  undoubtedly  atrophied,  and  added 
to  this  atrophy  there  was  an  atrophy  of  the  posterior  portion  of 
the  parietal  region.  If  frontal  lesions  in  themselves  were  espe- 
cially allied  to  the  delusion  formation,  it  is  to  be  expected  that 
the  more  extensive  atrophy  would  have  been  accompanied  by  this 
mental  condition.  Such,  however,  was  not  found.  In  these  cases, 
therefore,  delusions  appear  not  to  be  directly  associated  with  a 
particular  location  or  a  particular  degree  of  atrophy. 

Patient  29,  who  showed  only  a  slight  frontal  atrophy,  ex- 
hibited an  accurate  and  exact  memory.  Patient  35,  especially 
during  his  early  Hospital  residence,  was  also  'found  to  have  a  good 
memory,  and  although  patient  30  was  noted  as  having  an  imper- 
fect memory  at  the  time  of  his  admission,  it  will  be  noted  that 
according  to  the  case  history  his  memory  varied  from  "good" 
to  "poor"  during  his  later  Hospital  residence.  At  times  he  ap- 
peared to  have  a  very  good  grasp  upon  his  surroundings,  to 
know  the  events  of  his  past  life  as  well  as  other  events,  and  at 
other  times  he  did  not  appear  to  remember  these  things.  In 
the  other  cases  memory  was  poor.  In  the  cases  of  patients 
36  and  37  memory  for  remote  events,  especially  those  of  their 
childhood,  was  fair,  but  for  more  recent  occurrences  memory 
was  bad.  When  we  consider  these  phenomena  in  connection 
with  the  extent  and  degree  of  the  atrophies  which  the  brains 
of  these  patients  exhibited,  there  appears  to  be  no  correlation 
whatsoever. 

Case  33  was  the  only  patient  who  was  completely  oriented,  and 
this  is  particularly  noticeable  in  view  of  the  extent  and  degree  of 
atrophy  in  his  brain.  Nine  of  the  other  cases  were  completely 
disoriented  for  time,  for  place,  and  for  persons.  Patient  29, 
with  a  slight  frontal  atrophy,  was  disoriented  for  time,  and 
similarly  patient  35,  while  patient  38  was  disoriented  for  persons 
and  only  partially  disoriented  for  time  and  for  place.  Patient 
30  exhibited  the  same  kind  of  fluctuation  regarding  this  mental 
function  as  he  did  for  memory,  in  that  at  times  he  appeared  to 


72  SHEPHERD  IVORY  FRANZ 

be  completely  disoriented,  to  know  nothing  of  time  or  of  place 
or  of  persons,  and  again  to  have  an  excellent  grasp  upon  these 
things.  In  this  way  there  appeared  to  be  fluctuations  in  his 
mental  condition,  and  these  have,  as  far  as  can  be  determined, 
no  direct  correspondence  with  the  cerebral  lesions  in  his  case. 
The  abnormalities  in  this  field  do  not  apparently  depend  upon 
the  character  or  degree  of  the  atrophies,  the  opposing  evidence 
shown  in  case  33  alone  being  such  as  would  prevent  any  definite 
statement  regarding  a  relation. 

Nine  of  these  patients  lacked  insight.  One,  patient  34,  had 
fair  insight  into  his  condition,  whereas  the  other  four  cases  had 
what  might  be  termed  partial  insight  in  that,  in  answers  to 
questions,  they  gave  evidence  that  they  recognized  that  something 
was  wrong  with  them.  They  were  incapable  of  making  any 
kind  of  analysis,  and  in  at  least  some  of  the  cases  it  is  not 
certain  that  their  remarks  should  be  interpreted  as  evidence  of 
insight.  Thus,  patient  26  appeared  to  have  no  insight  into  his 
condition  except  that  he  remarked  at  one  time  that  if  the  ques- 
tions had  been  asked  him  a  month  previously,  he  would  have 
been  able  to  answer  them.  Patients  28  and  32  were  more  evi- 
dently in  a  condition  in  which  they  appreciated  that  something 
was  wrong  with  their  minds,  for  both  admitted  that  there  were 
"things  in  their  minds  which  were  not  right,"  and  that  their 
minds  were  affected  in  some  manner.  There  appears  to  be  some 
relation  between  the  degree  of  brain  shrinkage  and  general 
insight  in  that  in  every  case  in  which  there  was  a  slight  degree 
of  atrophy  insight  was  present,  partially  at  least.  Insight,  how- 
ever, as  related  to  the  extent  of  atrophy,  does  not  appear  to  have 
much  correlation,  although  of  the  five  frontal  cases  three  did 
exhibit  some  insight  into  their  condition,  while  of  the  anterior 
atrophies  only  two  exhibited  insight,  and  of  the  four  remain- 
ing cases  none  exhibited  insight  in  any  degree. 

Some  of  the  other  abnormalities  which  have  been  recorded 
in  the  case  histories  may  have  relations  to  the  sensory  defects 
which  were  present  in  these  patients,  although  this  is  not  ap- 
parent. Apprehensiveness,  or  delusions,  or  feelings  of  depression 
or  apathy  may  be  due  to  the  presence  of  defects  of  hearing, 


SYMPTOMATOLOGICAL  DIFFERENCES  IN  THE  INSANE      73 

which  abnormality  was  'found  in  eight  of  these  cases,  and  which 
in  otherwise  normal  individuals  appears  at  times  to  be  associated 
with  suspiciousness  and  allied  affective  states.  It  will  be  re- 
membered that  patient  28  had  some  impairment  of  the  skin 
sensations,  and  it  is  possible  that  his  hallucinations  and  his 
delusions  of  disorientation,  snow  on  the  ground,  etc.,  which 
have  been  described  in  a  previous  paragraph,  may  have  been 
due  to  the  hypoesthesia  for  skin  sensations.  At  the  same  time 
it  should  be  recalled  that  patient  27  and  patient  34  exhibited 
signs  of  beginning  cataract,  but  these  visual  defects  did  not 
bring  about  any  types  of  visual  hallucinations  or  delusions. 
It  is  unfortunate  that  the  "impairment  of  perception"  noted  in 
patient  30  is  not  more  fully  described,  for  it  is  possible  that 
there  were,  in  his  case,  sensory  abnormalities  which  may  have 
had  certain  relations  with  the  delusions  and  hallucinations  of 
which  this  patient  gave  evidence.  The  impairment  of  skin 
sensations  in  patient  28  is  not  correlated  with  atrophy  of  the 
postcentral  region,  since  it  will  be  remembered  that  this  patient 
exhibited  only  a  slight  atrophy  in  one  hemisphere. 

When  we  deal  with  these  patients  as  a  group,  we  find,  as  in 
the  other  psychoses,  extensive  atrophies  associated  with  some 
mental  conditions  quite  similar  to  those  found  in  the  patients 
with  the  less  extensive  atrophies.  In  general  it  may  be  said 
that  the  exaggerated  atrophies  have  given  rise  to  -no  more 
prominent  symptoms  than  the  milder  or  slight  degrees  of 
atrophy  which  were  recorded  as  being  present  in  the  brains 
of  three  of  these  patients. 


SUMMARY 

Although  all  the  cases  which  have  been  studied  exhibited 
atrophies  which  always  included  the  frontal  cerebral  convolu- 
tions, and  some  also  included  atrophies  of  the  neighboring 
central  and  parietal  portions  of  the  cerebrum,  no  one  symptom 
was  found  to  be  constant,  with  the  possible  exception  of  that 
complex  condition  which  is  called  dementia.  It  is  also  to  be  noted 
that  although  the  degree  of  atrophy  varied  from  "slight"  to 
"marked"  no  one  symptom  or  degree  of  symptom  was  found 
to  correspond  with  these  cerebral  changes.  In  general,  there- 
fore, we  may  say  that  there  is  no  apparent  correlation  between 
the  extent  or  the  degree  oi  atrophy  and  the  general  mental 
condition.  Those  individuals  who  exhibited  only  frontal 
atrophies  at  times  showed  as  much  dementia  as  those  in  which 
the  atrophy  was  more  extensive,  and  many  of  the  cases  in 
which  the  atrophy  was  of  a  slight  nature  were  also  as  demented 
as  those  in  which  the  atrophy  was  noted  to  be  great. 

Nor  does  there  appear  to  be  any  correlation  between  the 
form  of  the  disease  in  the  individual  groups  and  the  extent 
or  the  degree  of  the  cerebral  atrophy.  It  is  true  that  more 
of  the  dementia  precox  group  with  anterior  atrophies  were 
catatonic  in  nature  and  that  more  of  the  frontal  cases  appeared 
to  be  rather  paranoid.  On  the  other  hand,  both  with  frontal 
and  anterior  atrophies,  sufficient  cases  with  other  forms  of  the 
disease  were  observed,  and  it  seems  certain  that  the  extent 
of  the  atrophy  is  not  the  determining  element  in  the  production 
of  the  collection  of  symptoms  which  give  warrant  for  the 
diagnosis  of  the  "form"  of  the  disease.  For  the  arteriosclerotic, 
senile  and  paretic  groups  of  cases  a  similar  statement  may  be  made. 
In  none  of  these  collections  of  cases  did  there  appear  to  be 
any  definite  correlation  between  the  degree  of  the  atrophy  and 
the  special  form  of  the  disease,  and  in  the  special  discussions 
of  the  individual  groups  of  cases  it  has  been  stated  that  no 


SYMPTOM ATOLOG1CAL  DIFFERENCES  IN  THE  INSANE      75 

correlation  between  the  degree  of  dementia  (paretic,  arterio- 
sclerotic,  senile,  or  precox)  and  the  extent  or  the  degree  of  atrophy 
was  present. 

A  similar  statement  holds  true  for  the  probability  of  correla- 
tion between  degrees  of  atrophy  and  the  general  behavior  of 
the  cases  which  have  been  considered.  Such  behavior  as  the 
care  of  the  person  and  the  relations  of  the  individual  to  his 
environment  and  to  the  other  patients  did  not  vary  in  accordance 
with  the  location  or  the  amount  of  the  cerebral  changes. 

On  the  motor  side,  similarly,  we  find  no  definite  correlation 
existing.  In  the  dementia  precox  group  those  with  atrophies 
extending  beyond  the  .frontal  regions  differed  in  no  respect  from 
those  in  which  the  atrophy  was  confined  to  the  less  extensive 
area.  The  paretics  with  simple  frontal  lesions  were  perhaps 
more  active  than  those  with  the  lesions  extending  over  the 
central  convolutions  and  into  the  parietal  lobe,  but  the  cases 
of  senile  dementia  and  those  with  arteriosclerosis  /cerebralis 
were  about  equal  in  this  respect.  The  motor  phenomena  which 
are  found  associated  in  these  cases  with  atrophies  do  not  appear 
to  be  correlated  with  the  degree  of  the  atrophy.  In  none  of 
these  collections  of  cases  was  it  found  that  the  motor  phenomena 
differed  to  any  extent  when  the  degree  of  atrophy  was  great 
from  those  in  which  the  atrophy  was  slight. 

The  emotional  or  affective  states  of  these  patients  varied 
considerably.  No  one  fact  stands  out  particularly  to  indicate 
any  possible  relation  between  the  affective  states  of  these  pa- 
tients and  the  extent  or  the  degree  of  the  cerebral  atrophy,  and 
in  fact  in  this  case  the  conclusion  can  also  be  drawn  that  there 
is  no  correlation  of  this  character. 

The  variety  o;f  the  hallucinations  which  have  been  recorded 
and  the  fact  that  these  symptoms  were  not  uniformly  present 
in  those  with  similar  atrophies  does  not  indicate  any  definite 
relation  between  the  appearance  of  hallucinations  and  the 
atrophies  with  which  we  dealt.  It  would  appear  that  in 
the  dementia  precox  group  the  more  extensive  atrophies  were 
more  frequently  associated  with  the  presence  of  hallucinations, 


76  SHEPHERD  IVORY  FRANZ 

but  such  a  conclusion  can  not  be  made  regarding  the  groups  of 
paretic,  senile  and  arteriosclerotic  dements. 

Nor  do  the  delusions  appear  to  be  correlated  with  the  extent 
or  degree  of  atrophies.  Delusions  appear  to  be  as  relatively 
frequent  in  case  of  frontal  atrophy  as  in  those  cases  in  which 
the  atrophy  is  more  extensive,  and  in  this  connection  it  is  of 
interest  to  note  that  with  the  marked  changes  in  the  frontal 
lobes  which  have  been  noted  in  connection  with  a  number  of 
the  cases  delusions  were  not  always  found.  From  the  data 
which  were  available  to  him,  Southard  has  concluded  that  frontal 
lesions  are  more  frequently  accompanied  by  delusions,  but  in 
the  present  series  sufficient  cases  in  which  no  delusions  were 
present  have  been  observed,  in  which  frontal  atrophies  were 
present,  either  simple,  or  combined  with  those  of  the  central 
convolutions  and  at  times  with  those  of  the  parietal  region.  It 
is  worthy  of  note  that  df  the  dementia  precox  group  there 
were  three;  of  the  paretics,  one;  of  the  arteriosclerotics,  three; 
and  of  the  senile,  five ;  a  total  of  twelve  cases,  in  which  no  evidences 
of  delusions  was  discovered,  although  in  all  o*f  these  cases  an 
atrophy  of  the  frontal  lobes  was  discovered  at  the  time  of  the 
autopsy.  As  a  possible  definite  relation  with  the  cerebral  lesions 
it  may  be  said  that  most  of  the  patients  exhibited  poor  memory 
and  poor  orientation.  On  the  other  hand,  the  accuracy  of 
memory  of  some  of  the  patients  was  remarkable  in  view  of 
the  degree  of  dementia  which  was  noted  to  be  present,  and  in 
some  of  the  cases  the  orientation  ability  was  also  remarkable  in 
view  of  the  extensive  cerebral  changes  which  were  found. 

Summing  up  this  portion  we  may  definitely  say  that  there 
has  been  discovered  in  the  present  collection  of  38  cases  no 
definite  relation  between  (A)  the  degree  of  mentality,  or  lack 
of  mentality,  the  character  of  the  disease,  the  motor  and  affective 
states,  the  presence  of  delusions  or  hallucinations,  memory  or 
orientation  ability,  and  (B)  the  degree  and  extent  of  the  cerebral 
atrophies. 


GENERAL  DISCUSSION 

On  the  assumption  that  similar  portions  of  the  brain  in  differ- 
ent individuals  have  like  functions,  we  should  expect  that  corre- 
sponding lesions  would  give  rise  to  similar  symptoms.  That, 
however,  the  symptoms  differ  even  though  the  lesions  be  similar 
is  shown  by  the  study  o'f  the  case  histories  which  have  been 
summarized  in  previous  sections.  In  addition,  it  requires  no 
extensive  acquaintance  with  the  clinical  contributions  of  neurol- 
ogists to  recognize  that  cerebral  lesions  other  than  atrophies 
do  not  always  result  in  similar  mental  abnormalities,  or  symp- 
toms. Nor  does  it  require  any  great  amount  of  critical  ability 
to  reach  the  conclusion  that  numerous  facts  which  have  been 
recorded  in  clinical  neurological  literature  do  not  always  warrant 
simple  explanations  of  the  relation  of  cerebral  activities  and 
and  mental  phenomena.  It  is  apparent  that  the  variations  in  the 
symptoms  which  accompany  similar  cerebral  lesions  in  different 
individuals  have  often  been  minimized  and  sometimes  disre- 
garded, perhaps  'for  the  sake  of  simplicity  of  explanation. 

In  some  few  cases,  on  the  other  hand,  the  variations  in  the 
results  of  physiological  investigations  of  stimulation  and  ex- 
tirpation and  the  differences  in  the  clinical  symptoms  accompany- 
ing similar  cerebral  lesions  have  also  led  to  numerous  polemics. 
Dissimilar  symptoms  have  been  shamefully  taken  from  their 
settings,  estimated  too  highly  and  extravagantly  emphasized  by 
those  who  controverted  the  quasi-phrenological  views  of  cerebral 
'function. 

There  is,  however,  no  good  reason  either  for  disregarding 
the  dissimilarities  of  symptoms  accompanying  cerebral  lesions 
or  for  concluding  that  these  dissimilarities  demonstrate  that 
all  parts  of  the  brain  act  in  the  same  manner  and  that  there 
are  no  cerebral  "centers."  The  fact  of  dissimilarity  must  be 
accepted  and  brought  into  relation  with  the  other  facts  which 
are  known  regarding  cerebral  'function  and  control. 

It  has  already  been  pointed  out  that  in  physical  diseases  the 
symptoms  of  individuals  may  differ  to  a  considerable  extent, 
and  also  that  lesions  in  widely  separated  portions  of  the  brain 


78  SHEPHERD  IVORY  FRANZ 

may  give  rise  to  different  symptoms  in  different  individuals. 
Many  neurologists  now  recognize  the  latter  fact,  and  von 
Monakow  in  particular  has  been  insisting  that  this  fact  is  of 
primary  importance  'for  deducing  the  functions  and  functional 
connections  of  the  cerebrum.  The  similarity  of  symptoms  ac- 
companying different  lesions  is  readily  understood  if  we  conceive 
of  the  nervous  system,  and  especially  the  cerebrum,  as  a  collec- 
tion of  cells  with  connections,  the  function  of  which  is  fixed 
largely  because  of  the  intercommunicating  connections.  The 
individual  cell  has  its  own  function,  but  in  the  production  or 
control  of  any  of  the  cerebrally  produced  or  controlled  processes 
in  other  parts  o'f  the  body,  or  of  mental  states,  it  is  the  serial 
or  grouped  activities  of  nerve  cells  which  must  be  dealt  with. 
When  this  view  is  taken  it  becomes  clear  why  dissimilar  lesions 
may  produce  similar  symptoms.  A  break  at  any  part  of  the 
chain  will  prevent  the  normal  function,  which  is  serial. 

It  is  now  recognized  that  widely  separated  areas  are  always 
involved  even  when  only  a  very  small  portion  of  the  cerebral 
cortex  is  injured  or  destroyed.  The  destruction  of  some  por- 
tions may  immediately  produce  perfectly  obvious  de'fects  or  ex- 
aggerations of  normal  behavior,  while  the  destruction  of  other 
portions  may  give  rise  to  effects  which  are  not  immediate  or 
obvious.  The  effects  of  the  latter  lesions  are  sometimes  to  be 
discovered  only  indirectly  and  at  other  times  they  are,  as  von 
Monakow  points  out,  of  a  "latent  nature  and  only  become  mani- 
fest when  there  is  added  to  the  primary  operation  (i.e.,  a  lesion) 
a  second  operation  upon,  or  a  pathological  process  in,  another 
region  o'f  the  cortex."5 

The  present  series  of  facts  are,  however,  different  from  those 
with  which  von  Monakow  and  others  have  dealt.  They  are 
the  reverse  conditions,  and  they  are  not  readily  understandable 
on  the  hypothesis  which  von  Monakow  has  set  forth  to  explain 
the  similarity  of  symptoms  with  dissimilar  lesions.  In  connec- 
tion with  these  studies  of  'frontal  and  anterior  atrophies  it 
should  be  pointed  out  that  dissimilarities  of  symptoms  from 

*  Monakow,  C.  von.  Theoretische  Betrachtungen  iiber  die  Localisation  im 
Zentralnervensystem,  insbesonder  im  Grosshirn.  Ergebnisse  der  Physiol.,. 
1913,  13,  206-278. 


SYMPTOM ATOLOGICAL  DIFFERENCES  IN  THE  INSANE      79 

frontal  lesions  have  not  infrequently  been  the  cause  of  much 
discussion  and  recrimination.  Thus,  the  clinical  findings  which 
have  been  recorded  at  different  times  by  different  investigators 
whose  material  (patients)  was  not  exactly  the  same  have  given 
rise  to  the  hypothesis  that  the  frontal  lobes  are  (i)  intellectual 
centers,  (2)  centers  for  emotional  states,  (3)  centers  for  in- 
hibition, and  (4)  motor  centers.  Without  attempting  at  the 
present  time  to  advance  a  more  general  hypothesis  than  those 
already  suggested,  it  may  be  said  that  the  clinical  facts  are  not 
mutually  exclusive  and  that  all  can  be  brought  together  under 
one  heading  which  gives  a  suggestion  for  the  explanation  of  the 
functions  of  the  frontal  lobes. 

Facts  of  an  experimental  nature  are  also  at  hand  to  show 
that  the  definiteness  of  control  by  the  motor  (precentral)  cortex 
is  not  as  great  as  that  which  is  usually  assumed  to  be  the  case. 
These  will  be  dealt  with  in  the  second  part  of  this  monograph.  All 
of  the  facts  indicate  that  we  must  conclude  that  the  action  of  the 
cerebrum  is  a  diffuse  activity,  and  an  activity  which  varies  from 
individual  to  individual,  and  in  the  same  individual  from  time 
to  time.  A  full  discussion  of  this  hypothesis  will  appear  in 
connection  with  the  study  of  the  variability  of  control  from  the 
motor  cortex.  At  the  present  time  it  may  be  mentioned  that  it 
seems  most  satisfactory  to  consider  the  cerebrum  as  a  labile 
organ  or,  in  other  words,  as  a  series  of  cells  with  numerous 
possibilities  of  connections.  The  number  of  connections  makes 
possible  a  variety  of  activities,  since  at  one  time  a  cell  may  act 
through  its  main  axonal  connection  upon  a  second  cell,  and  at 
another  time  through  its  collateral  connections  it  may  act  upon 
a  third  or  fourth  cell.  Probably  the  cerebral  cells  do  not  always 
discharge,  or  influence  other  cells,  in  the  same  manner.  We 
should,  therefore,  not  hold  to  a  belief  in  a  definiteness  and 
simplicity  of  physiological  connections,  but  rather  to  a  manifold- 
ness  of  connections,  any  one  or  more  of  which  may  be  used  or 
not  used  at  one  time.  The  use,  non-use,  or  disuse  of  any  one 
of  the  possible  paths  at  different  times  will  produce  differences 
in  behavior,  and  similar  lesions  of  cells  may,  therefore  produce 
different  symptoms  in  accordance  with  the  normal  (to  the  in- 
dividual) connections  which  have  thus  been  interfered  with. 


II 

VARIATIONS  IN  DISTRIBUTION  OF  THE 
MOTOR  CENTERS 

INTRODUCTION 

The  earliest  positive  results  of  the  electrical  stimulation  of 
the  cortex  were  obtained  by  Fritsch  and  Hitzig,  and  at  that 
time  and  for  a  number  of  years  the  movements  obtained  on 
stimulation  of  the  cerebral  cortex  were  described  in  terms  such 
as  to  suggest  that  the  resulting  movements  were  definite  move- 
ments of  individual  muscles.  Subsequently  attempts  were  made 
to  determine  the  cortical  localization  or  representation  of  in- 
dividual muscles  or  muscle  groups,  but  it  was  amply  demon- 
strated by  more  recent  experiments  (especially  those  of 
Sherrington  and  Griinbaum  (6)  that  all  the  movements  which 
result  from  cortical  stimulation  are  complex  movements.  These 
are  "movements"  in  a  particular  sense  of  the  word  and  not 
simply  contractions  of  muscles.  They  are  movements  which 
are  best  described  as  behavior  phenomena,  not  only  movements 
of  flexion  but  movements  of  grasping,  not  simply  extension 
movements  but  movements  of  repulsion  or  thrusting  away.  In 
many  cases  the  complex  activities  of  the  associated  muscular 
contractions  can  best,  and  at  times  can  only,  be  described  in 
terms  of  such  behavior  activities. 

From  time  to  time  in  attempting  to  demonstrate  the  motor 
control  by  the  cortex  by  stimulating  the  so-called  motor  centers 
difficulty  was  experienced  by  me  with  some  animals  in  obtaining 
special  reactions  of  parts  which  appeared  to  be  readily  obtainable 
in  other  animals.1  These  variations  in  the  stimulability  or  in 
the  control  from  the  cortex  were  at  first,  on  account  of  the 

1  Compare  also  Sherrington's  remark :  "In  the  cat  it  is  in  my  experience 
quite  infrequent  to  obtain  primary  extension  of  the  crossed  elbow  from  the 
cortex.  Flexion  is  readily  and  regularly  obtained."  Integvative  Action  of 
the  Nervous  System,  1906,  page  293. 


VARIATIONS  IN  DISTRIBUTION  OF  MOTOR  CENTERS       81 

definiteness  with  which  most  previous  results  have  usually  been 
described,  believed  by  me  to  be  due  to  defects  in  technic  or  to 
variations  in  the  excitability  of  different  cortical  regions  in  dif- 
ferent animals  under  anaesthesia.  A  careful  examination  of 
the  literature  devoted  to  the  experimental  investigation  of  the 
motor  cortex  in  animals  indicated,  however,  that  notwithstanding 
the  supposed  definiteness  there  was  considerable  variation  in 
the  location  of  special  areas  in  different  animals.  This  variation 
appeared  to  depend,  to  some  extent  at  least,  upon  the  varying 
configurations  of  the  fissures  and  consequently  the  varying  posi- 
tions of  the  collections  of  cells  governing  particular  movements. 
On  the  other  hand,  it  also  appeared  possible  that  some  of  these 
variations  might  be  variations  of  a  primary  nature,  i.e.,  not 
due  to  the  adventitious  condition  of  anaesthesia  or  other  similar 
circumstances.  In  fact,  the  careful  examination  of  published 
work  on  the  motor  cortex,  especially  that  of  the  Vogts  (H), 
shows  that  the  location  of  the  individual  cerebral  areas  controlling 
certain  of  the  small  bodily  segments  is  not  as  anatomically 
(spatially)  definite  as  has  been  supposed.  Such  as  least  was  the 
suggestion  that  was  received  when  the  results  of  previous  in- 
vestigations were  compared.  Accurate  determinations  of  the 
variations  of  previous  investigations  were  not  found  to  be  prac- 
tical, both  on  account  of  the  differences  in  method  that  individual 
investigations  had  employed  and  their  methods  of  recording 
results  and  also  on  account  of  the  different  species  of  animals 
on  which  the  tests  were  made. 

Because  of  these  considerations,  it  was  thought  advisable  to 
attempt  comparisons  of  the  results  of  the  stimulation  of  the 
brains  of  a  number  of  animals  of  the  same  species  in  order  to 
determine  how  much,  if  any,  variation  there  is  in  the  relation 
of  parts  of  the  precentral  cortex  to  the  control  of  different  bodily 
segments.  It  appeared  possible  that  in  this  area  of  the  brain 
there  are  variations  of  an  individual  nature,  not  explainable  on 
the  ground  of  variations  in  fissural  configurations.  The  animals 
chosen  for  the  present  investigation  were  monkeys,  macacus 
rhesus,  five  of  which  gave  results  of  value. 


METHODS 

An  animal  was  driven  from  its  cage  into  a  large  burlap  sack 
and  completely  anaesthetized.  It  was  then  transferred  to  the 
operating  board  and  kept  under  the  anaesthetic  during  the  whole 
period  of  the  experiment  The  A.C.E.  mixture  was  used  in 
all  of  the  experiments.  At  the  time  the  movements  of  the 
animal  were  to  be  determined  all  the  limbs  were  untied,  but 
the  head  was  kept  steadied  in  a  head-holder  so  that  head  move- 
ments might  not  take  place  or  be  minimized.  The  present  series 
of  tests  were  intended  to  deal  largely  with  the  relation  of  the 
cerebrum  to  the  leg  and  arm  movements,  and  the  head  move- 
ments were  disregarded  except  when  the  latter  were  associated 
with  movements  of  the  other  segments.  Since,  however,  the 
head  was  kept  fairly  rigid,  most  of  the  head  movements  were 
prevented  and  with  the  exception  of  a  few  have  not  been  re- 
corded. This  procedure  of  steadying  the  head  were  also  used 
'for  another  reason,  viz.,  to  prevent  possible  injuries  to  the 
brain  when  the  head  moved  at  the  time  the  stimulating  electrodes 
were  applied.  Except  by  having  the  electrodes  attached  firmly  to 
the  skull  so  that  they  move  with  it,  it  is  not  always  possible  to 
avoid  accidental  injuries  of  this  nature,  but  in  the  present  series 
such  avoidable  injuries  to  the  brain  were  prevented  by  keeping 
the  head  steadied. 

A  one-half  inch  trephine  was  used  to  cut  through  the  skull 
to  such  a  point  that  the  button  of  bone  could  be  readily  removed. 
The  trephine  was  not  permitted  to  cut  entirely  through  the  inner 
table  of  the  skull  on  account  of  the  possibility  that  the  trephine 
teeth  might  also  cut  through  the  dura  mater  and  thus  injure 
the  brain.  In  this  way  also  injury  to  the  cerebral  cortex  was 
prevented  as  much  as  possible,  and  in  no  case,  as  far  as  could 
be  determined  by  an  inspection  of  the  brain  through  a  magnify- 
ing glass  was  any  gross  injury  produced.  After  the  skull  open- 
ings had  been  made  in  this  manner  they  were  enlarged  in  different 


VARIATIONS  IN  DISTRIBUTION  OF  MOTOR  CENTERS       83 

directions  by  bone  forceps  in  order  to  uncover  a  large  portion 
of  the  frontal  area,  all  of  the  so-called  precentral  region  and 
a  considerable  part  of  the  postcentral  cortex.  Bleeding  from 
the  diploe  was  checked  by  the  application  of  bone  wax.  When 
the  large  openings  on  both  sides  of  the  skull  had  been  made 
the  bridge  of  bone  which  covered  the  longitudinal  fissure  was 
cut  through  after  ligatures  had  been  placed  at  both  extremities 
of  the  bridge  in  order  to  prevent  bleeding  from  the  longitudinal 
sinus.  In  previous  tests  bleeding  from  this  source  was  found 
to  be  very  great  and  in  one  of  the  animals  used  in  the  present 
series  even  the  precautions  of  ligating  the  longitudinal  sinus 
which  were  taken  did  not  prevent  death  from  hemorrhage.  The 
dura  was  next  cut  and  this  was  partly  reflected  and  partly  cut 
away  so  as  to  leave  the  portion  of  the  cortex  which  was  to  be 
stimulated  bare. 

The  part  of  the  brain  which  was  not  at  the  time  being  sub- 
jected to  stimulation  exploration  was  covered  with  thin  rubber 
tissue  which  had  been  wet  with  warm  normal  salt  solution  and 
all  was  covered  with  a  large  sponge  of  cotton  which  had  also 
been  wet  and  warmed  in  the  same  solution.  Sufficient  time 
elapsed  between  the  individual  stimuli  to  permit  the  recording 
of  results  and  this  also  rested  the  brain  tissue.  After  a  series 
of  half  a  dozen  or  more  stimulations  the  part  of  the  brain  which 
was  being  used  was  covered  by  the  rubber  tissue  and  the  sponge, 
and  the  brain  was  permitted  to  rest  for  a  longer  period,  five 
to  ten  minutes.  In  this  way  the  brain  was  protected  at  times 
when  the  tests  were  not  being  made.  In  this  way  also  drying 
could  not  take  place,  and  little,  if  any,  of  the  anaesthetic  gases 
escaping  into  the  room  could  act  directly  upon  the  cerebral  cells. 
At  the  same  time  the  use  of  the  rubber  tissue  prevented  too  much 
moistening  of  the  brain  substance,  for  it  should  be  recognized 
that  there  is  a  possibility  that  the  use  of  too  much  of  the 
normal  salt  solution  may,  by  osmosis,  sufficiently  change  the 
chemical  character  of  the  cortical  cells  to  produce  alterations 
in  their  irritability.  The  duration  of  the  tests  on  one  side  of 
the  brain  was  usually  about  three  hours.  In  certain  cases  where 
the  number  of  stimulable  points  was  small  this  time  was  short- 


$4  SHEPHERD  IVORY  FRANZ 

ened  and  on  one  animal  in  which  the  number  of  points  was 
large,  this  time  was  much  increased.  In  addition  to  the  careful 
administration  of  the  anaesthetic,  the  precautions  to  avoid 
"fatigue"  and  those  to  prevent  osmotic  changes  from  the  ap- 
plication of  the  moistening  solutions  tended  to  keep  the  cortical 
irritability  at  approximately  constant  level,  and  any  experiment, 
if  it  had  appeared  necessary  or  advisable,  might  have  been  carried 
on  for  a  greater  length  of  time. 

It  was  not  .found  difficult  to  keep  the  animal  sufficiently  under 
the  effect  of  the  anaesthetic  during  the  whole  period  of  time, 
for  care  was  taken  that  the  quantity  should  be  sufficient  to 
prevent  any  voluntary  movements  which  might  mask  or  inter- 
fere with  the  movements  which  were  produced  by  the  electrical 
stimulations,  but  at  the  same  time  the  anaesthetic  was  not  pushed 
to  such  a  degree  as  would  be  necessary  in  experiments  in  which 
complete  relaxation  of  the  involuntary  muscles  is  desired.  In 
no  case  was  the  anaesthetic  deep  enough  to  produce  a  relaxation 
of  the  sphincters,  but  by  continuous  careful  application  all  of 
the  voluntary  movements  were  prevented,  even  at  the  times 
when  cutting  and  trephining  were  performed. 

The  cortex  was  stimulated  by  the  bipolar  method,  the  in- 
duction coil  being  a  standard  Helmholtz  apparatus.  The 
platinum  points  were  separated  by  approximately  one-quarter 
of  a  millimeter  and  each  of  the  points  was  approximately  of 
the  same  size.  The  distance,  therefore,  between  the  centers  of 
the  points  was  approximately  one-half  of  a  millimeter.  The 
electrode  points  were  applied  to  a  special  portion  of  the  cortex 
and  after  the  stimulation  and  the  resulting  reaction  (when  any 
occurred)  they  were  removed.  The  secondary  coil  of  the  in- 
ductorium  was  arranged  at  the  beginning  of  a  series  of  tests 
on  an  animal  at  such  a  point  as  to  give  a  supra-minimal  stimula- 
tion. It  was  kept  at  this  point  throughout  the  series  of  tests 
on  that  animal,  with  the  exception  that  when  with  this  strength 
of  current  an  apparently  non-stimulable  area  was  found,  the 
strength  of  the  stimulus  was  increased  to  determine  whether  the 
failure  of  reaction  was  due  to  a  normal  non-stimulable  character 
of  the  special  area  or  to  a  decrease  of  irritability.  It  was  found 


VARIATIONS  IN  DISTRIBUTION  OF  MOTOR  CENTERS       85 

that  the  increased  stimulus  seldom  gave  rise  to  movements,  and 
when  failures  with  the  increased  stimulus  were  encountered 
it  was  concluded  that  these  areas  were  normally  non-stimulable. 
It  is  likely  that  further  increases  in  the  strength  of  the  stimuli 
might  have  resulted  in  reactions,  but  with  greater  strengths  of 
stimuli  the  possibility  of  "spreading"  is  greater  and  the  results 
would  have  been  open  to  serious  objection  on  this  score. 

The  stimulations  were  usually  first  begun  at  the  upper  portion 
of  the  precentral  area  where  the  motor  cortex  dip  downwards 
into  the  longitudinal  fissure.  Eight  or  ten  stimuli  were  given  in  a 
regular  order  as  close  as  possible  to  the  central  fissure,  and  ex- 
tending serially  towards  the  fissure  of  Sylvius,  the  stimulated 
points  being  approximately  one  millimeter  apart.  Another  series 
was  begun  at  the  extreme  upper  limit  of  the  precentral  area  and 
the  stimuli  were  carried  downwards  in  a  line  which  was  approxi- 
mately one  millimeter  in  front  of  the  line  along  which  the  pre- 
vious series  of  stimuli  had  been  given.  Third,  fourth,  etc.,  series 
were  made  in  the  same  manner,  each  extending  one  millimeter 
anteriorly  to  the  previous  one.  In  this  way  the  experiment  was 
carried  on  until  in  passing  forwards  a  line  or  series  of  stimuli 
had  been  given  which  produced  no  observable  reactions.  In  cer- 
tain animals,  on  account  of  the  presence  of  blood  vessels  and 
extra  fissures,  lines  in  which  no  stimuli  were  given  were  present. 
In  most  cases,  however,  the  blood  vessels  which  were  encountered 
were  not  much  greater  than  one  millimeter  in  diameter  and  thus 
the  general  experimental  relationships  were  not  markedly  dis- 
turbed. After  such  an  area,  about  ten  millimeters  in  length  and 
in  width  to  correspond  to  the  stimulable  zone,  had  been  carefully 
examined,  similar  series  were  made  in  the  area  immediately  below 
(towards  the  fissure  of  Sylvius).  In  this  manner  the  whole  of 
the  superficial  precentral  cortex  extending  from  the  longitudinal 
fissure  downwards  as  far  as  the  head  area  was  carefully  mapped 
out. 

After  every  second  stimulation  a  small  portion  of  a  mixture 
which  was  composed  of  vaseline  and  analine-black  was  applied 
to  the  point  which  had  been  previously  stimulated.  This  mix- 
ture was  viscid,  and  sufficient  could  be  taken  upon  the  point  of 


86  SHEPHERD  IVORY  FRANZ 

a  sharp  probe  which,  when  lightly  touched  to  the  brain,  left  a 
speck  of  this  "paint"  upon  the  brain  surface.  In  some  cases  the 
individual  points  did  not  stand  out  by  this  method  as  clearly 
as  was  desired,  but  in  many  of  the  experiments  the  points  of 
application  of  the  analine-black  mixture  could  be  correctly  de- 
termined and  the  relative  locations  of  the  individual  points  of 
stimulation  were  therefore  accurately  given  on  the  cortex.  The 
application  of  the  wet  rubber  tissue  and  sponge  after  a  series  of 
tests  tended  to  obliterate  the  marks  which  were  made,  but  the 
pia  mater  (and  the  brain?)  was  sufficiently  stained  in  many  cases 
so  that  the  individual  points  were  to  be  seen  when  the  brain  was 
examined  with  a  magnifier.  The  stained  points  were  later  com- 
pared with  the  photographs  and  with  the  diagrams  which  were 
used. 

At  the  time  of  the  performance  of  the  experiment  a  rough 
sketch  (with  a  magnification  of  about  ten  diameters)  of  the 
general  appearance  of  the  precentral  and  postcentral  regions  of 
the  hemispheres  was  made.  On  this  sketch  the  longitudinal  fis- 
sures, the  central  fissures,  subsidiary  fissures,  and  the  blood 
vessels  were  noted.  Following  each  test  the  location  of  the  stimu- 
lated point  was  marked  on  the  sketch  so  that  this  could  be  com- 
pared with  the  location  of  the  stain  which  had  been  applied  to 
the  brain.  The  serial  numbers  of  the  tests  were  noted  on  the 
sketch  in  approximately  correct  relations.  These  sketches  were 
subsequently  used  for  the  identification  of  the  stimulated  points. 
After  the  removal  of  the  brain  and  its  hardening  in  formalin 
(10  per  cent)  the  area  which  was  found  to  be  stimulable  was 
again  sketched  by  placing  over  the  cortex  a  piece  of  transparent 
paper  which  was  pressed  down  tightly  and  which  was  marked 
to  show  all  of  the  points  of  interest  (fissures,  blood  vessels,  ex- 
tent  of  the  stained  zone,  etc.)  in  that  particular  area.  These 
diagrams  were  then  placed  in  the  Leitz  projection  apparatus  and 
drawings  were  made  of  the  results  with  a  magnification  of  ten 
diameters.  On  the  drawings  which  accompany  the  present  work 
corrections  have  been  made  in  the  diagrams  by  comparisons  with 
actual  brain  pictures  (photographs),  so  that  the  diagrams  which 
are  here  represented  are  combinations  of  the  sketches  taken  at 


VARIATIONS  IN  DISTRIBUTION  OF  MOTOR  CENTERS       87 

the  time  of  the  performance  of  the  experiments  with  the  actual 
pictures  of  the  brains.  At  the  time  the  brain  was  photographed 
a  two  millimeter  scale  was  placed  along  the  central  fissure  and 
photographed  with  the  brain.  The  photographs  were  later  mag- 
nified uniformly  at  10  diameters,  each  millimeter  on  the  photo- 
graphed scale  corresponding  to  10  on  the  diagram,  and 
drawings  made  of  all  the  important  landmarks.2 

On  these  drawings  the  points  of  stimulation  were  noted  and  the 
numbers  corresponding  to  the  serial  tests  were  recorded.  Since 
the  diagrams  were  drawn  to  scale,  the  results  which  are  recorded 
in  them  may  be  considered  to  represent  fairly  accurately  the  ex- 
tents of  the  stimulable  motor  areas  in  the  individual  hemispheres 
of  the  five  monkeys  which  were  used.  It  will  be  noted  that  these 
areas  differ  considerably  in  absolute  sizes,  monkey  2  having  a 
more  extensive  responsive  area  than  any  of  the  other  animals, 
monkey  4  having  the  least  extensive  stimulable  motor  area. 

At  the  time  the  experiments  were  performed  there  were  usually 
four  observers.  The  chief  duty  of  one  was  the  manipulation  of 
the  electrodes.  This  observer  also  directed  the  experiment,  noting 
on  the  rough  sketch  the  points  which  had  been  stimulated  and 

3  Since  there  is  a  considerable  curvature  of  the  brain  from  the  longitudinal 
fissure  towards  the  temporal  areas  (of  special  interest  in  this  connection 
being  the  curvature  in  the  region  of  the  central  fissure)  the  photographs 
showed  considerable  spatial  distortions  of  the  sides  of  the  brain.  In  the 
projection  of  the  photographs  only  the  two  millimeter  divisions  on  the 
relatively  flat  superior  surface  of  the  brain  could  be  made  to  correspond  with 
the  twenty  millimeter,  divisions  of  the  projection  screen.  The  landmarks 
(•fissures,  blood  vessels,  etc.)  and  the  scale  divisions  were  recorded  as  they 
were  magnified.  These  drawings  were  then  dealt  with  as  if  the  curved 
surface  was  simple,  i.e.,  like  that  of  a  cylinder,  and  the  proper  geometrical 
projections  were  made  to  bring  about  the  uniform  distribution  of  the  area. 
It  is  realized  that  for  the  most  accurate  representation  we  should  deal  not 
only  with  the  superior-inferior  curvature  but  with  the  fronto-posterior  cur- 
vature as  well.  The  latter  curvature  in  the  region  of  the  central  fissure  is 
however  slight  as  compared  with  the  superior-inferior  curvature  and  has 
been  disregarded.  The  error  of  recording  due  to  this  method  is,  I  am 
advised,  probably  not  as  large  as  5  per  cent.  Since  also  these  drawings 
were  later  compared  with  the  brains,  with  the  enlarged  diagrams  on  trans- 
parent paper,  and  with  the  original  sketches,  the  error  is  doubtless  much  less 
than  this  amount.  It  is  presumed,  however,  that  the  error  is  about  equal 
for  all  brains  and  the  results  are,  therefore,  comparable. 


88  SHEPHERD  IVORY  FRANZ 

applying  to  the  cortex  the  analine-black  mixture.  A  second  ob- 
server took  charge  of  the  anaesthetic,  his  attention  being  directed 
principally  to  the  matter  of  keeping  the  animal  under  good  anaes- 
thetic control,  but  he  assisted  at  times  in  the  observation  of  the 
activities  of  the  animal  which  accompanied  the  stimulations.  A 
third  observer  recorded  the  results  of  the  individual  stimuli  on 
sheets  of  paper,  noting  at  the  same  time  the  serial  numbers  and  the 
times  of  the  stimulations.  This  observer  also  cooperated  with 
the  fourth  one  in  noting  the  character  of  the  movements.  The 
fourth  observer  had  as  his  sole  duty  the  observation  of  the  move- 
ments which  resulted  from  the  stimuli  and  the  description  of 
the  movement  so  that  they  might  be  recorded.  As  has  already 
been  said,  the  anaesthetist  also  occasionally  helped  to  observe 
the  movements.  Only  when  the  two  or  three  special  observers  of 
the  reactions  (movements)  were  satisfied  regarding  the  charac- 
ters of  the  reactions  were  they  recorded.  If  there  was  a  doubt  of 
a  more  or  less  serious  nature  regarding  the  combination  of  move- 
ments which  was  produced  the  particular  point  was  subjected  to 
a  second  stimulation  at  a  later  time. 

Following  the  determination  of  all  the  available  points  in  the 
leg  and  arm  areas  the  animal  was  killed  by  an  overdose  of  chloro- 
form. The  brains  were  hardened  in  formalin  and  preserved  for 
future  study.  The  extent  of  the  stimulable  areas  was  determined 
in  the  manner  described  above  after  the  brain  had  been  thoroughly 
hardened.  The  location  of  the  points  for  the  individual  segments 
or  parts  of  segments  was  also  made  in  the  manner  similar  to  that 
described  above,  and  are  here  reproduced  in  the  diagrams. 

The  serial  numbers  of  the  original  individual  tests  have  not  been 
included  in  the  present  accounts,  for  to  bring  about  an  areal  cor- 
respondence of  the  allied  areas  in  all  animals  the  serial  numbers 
in  the  individual  experiments  were  not  available.  This  was  due 
to  the  fact  that  variations  in  the  conduct  of  the  experiments,  i.e., 
the  varying  location  of  the  different  fissures  and  different  blood 
vessels,  etc.,  were  present  to  alter  the  constant  serial  character  of 
the  experiments  in  the  different  hemispheres.3  Furthermore,  in 

*That  is,  test  10,  or  test  36,  or  test  72,  did  not  always  bear  the  same  rela- 
tion to  the  well  known  anatomical  landmarks  in  all  hemispheres. 


VARIATIONS  IN  DISTRIBUTION  OF  MOTOR  CENTERS       89 

the  illustrations  which  are  given  later  the  areas  have  been,  for 
the  purpose  of  comparison,  divided  into  special  zones,  although  it 
should  be  understood  that  these  zones  by  the  methods  used,  are 
only  approximately  determinable  for  any  one  hemisphere.4  At 
the  same  time,  since  the  individual  areas  differ  widely  in  extent,  it 
should  be  noted  that  spatial  comparisons  of  two  or  more  hemi- 
spheres can  be  only  approximate. 

In  the  present  work  as  has  been  noted  above,  attention  was 
directed  to  those  areas  which  are  concerned  with  the  movements 
of  the  leg  (including  the  thigh,  lower  leg,  foot  and  toes)  and 
with  the  arm  (including  the  shoulder,  forearm,  hand  and  fin- 
gers). The  associated  movements  of  the  head  which  were  occa- 
sionally met  with  in  the  stimulation  of  the  arm  area  will  not  be 
detailed  in  the  present  paper.  The  present  paper,  therefore,  deals 
primarily  with  the  areas  concerned  with  the  anterior  and  posterior 
limbs  and  occasionally  with  the  associated  movements  of  the  tail 
The  results  of  the  experiments  are  given  in  four  divisions  as  fol- 
lows :  ( i )  the  total  extents  of  the  stimulable  areas  for  the  arm  and 
leg;  (2)  a  comparison  of  the  distribution  for  the  leg  and  for  the 
arm;  (3)  a  comparison  of  the  distributions  of  the  areas  for  the 
smaller  segments  (ringers,  hand,  lower  arm,  upper  arm,  toes,  foot, 
lower  leg  and  thigh)  ;  and  (4)  the  anomalous  distribution  of  the 
stimulable  areas,  namely  (a)  those  areas  which  gave  leg  move- 
ments when  the  surrounding  areas  gave  arm  movements,  (b) 
those  areas  which  gave  arm  movements  when  the  surrounding 
areas  gave  leg  movements,  and  (c)  the  nonstimulable  (or  rela- 
tively non-stimulable)  areas  which  were  surrounded  by  readily 
stimulable  areas. 

Several  possible  objections  may  be  urged  against  considering  the 
diagrams  to  include  all  of  the  motor  responsive  areas  for  the  arm 

*  In  other  words,  in  the  diagrams  each  point  of  experimental  stimulation  is 
represented  by  an  area.  Since  the  stimulated  points  were  one  millimeter 
apart,  the  square  millimeter  surrounding  a  point  was  considered  to  correspond 
with  the  point.  A  micrometric  method  of  moving  the  stimulating  electrodes 
or  the  use  of  one  pole,  by  the  unipolar  and  monopolar  methods,  would  have 
permitted  the  stimulation  of  more  points,  and  the  diagrammatic  representa- 
tions would  have  been  nearer  the  actual  conditions.  Since,  however,  we  deal 
with  comparative  results  with  the  same  method  on  all  animals  the  slight 
variations  due  to  method  can  be  largely  disregarded. 


90  SHEPHERD  IVORY  FRANZ 

and  leg.  One  possible  objection  is  that  the  stimuli  may  not  have 
been  sufficiently  strong  to  bring  about  responses  in  the  outlying 
portions  of  the  areas,  and  that  in  any  one  of  the  monkeys  the 
total  area  which  was  found  stimulable  may  be  only  the  combin- 
ation of  more  readily  stimulated  points.  Such  an  objection  would 
be  valid  were  it  not  that  in  those  cases  in  which  the  extent  of  the 
area  seemed  to  be  slight,  additional  slightly  stronger  stimuli  were 
given  in  order  that  there  should  be  a  reasonable  certainty  regard- 
ing the  outer  limits.  When  these  increased  stimuli  failed  to 
produce  a  response  it  was  deemed  that  the  limits  of  the  normally 
excitable  area  had  been  reached.  The  use  of  much  stronger 
stimuli  may  be  objected  to  in  this  connection,  since  the  stronger 
stimuli  tend  to  spread  to  a  great  degree  and,  therefore,  to  have 
a  more  widespread  physiological  effect  than  the  weaker  stimuli. 
Since  the  precautions  were  taken  however,  as  a  check  and  nega- 
tive results  were  obtained,  the  negative  results  may  be  considered 
to  be  confirmations  of  the  limitation  of  what  we  may  call  the 
"immediately"  excitable  zone. 

Another  objection  which  may  be  urged  against  considering 
these  results  of  absolute  (rather  than  relative)  value  is  that  no  ac- 
count has  been  taken  of  the  stimulable  portions  of  the  cortex 
which  are  included  within  the  central  and  within  the  subsidiary 
fissures.  This  objection  is  in  most  respects  weighty  for  it  is 
realized  that  there  may  be  a  possible  inverse  relation  between  the 
amount  of  the  stimulable  cortex  on  the  convexity  or  surface  of 
the  brain  and  that  to  be  found  lying  within  the  fissures.  It  may 
be  admitted  at  the  outset  that  in  the  present  work  no  measure- 
ments (stimulation  or  otherwise)  have  been  made  of  the  quantity 
or  extent  of  the  motor  cortex  which  dips  down  into  the  central 
fissures.  It  may  also  be  admitted  that  some  of  the  variations 
which  have  been  discovered  by  the  present  methods  may  be  varia- 
tions of  an  anatomical  nature  as  described  above  (inverse  rela- 
tion of  surface  and  fissure  extents).  It  is  not  true,  however,  that 
all  the  variations  can  be  explained  in  this  manner.  Some  varia- 
tions that  will  be  noted  later,  especially  those  of  the  distribution 
of  the  areas  for  the  individual  segments  cannot  be  explained 
in  this  manner.  On  account  of  the  possible  objection  to  certain  of 


VARIATIONS  IN  DISTRIBUTION  OF  MOTOR  CENTERS        91 

the  results  it  may  be  mentioned  that  the  correlation  of  the  extent 
of  the  motor  cortex  anterior  to  and  that  within  the  fissure  of 
Rolando  is  a  problem  which  I  hope  to  be  able  to  deal  with  in  a 
subsequent  publication. 

A  third  objection  is  that  the  motor  areas  dipping  downward 
into  the  longitudinal  sulcus  have  not  been  considered.  In  most 
of  the  hemispheres  this  objection  does  not  hold  since  the  areas 
within  the  longitudinal  sulcus  were  investigated  and  the  results 
recorded.  In  the  case  of  the  left  hemisphere  of  monkey  3,  how- 
ever, not  even  all  the  convexity  surface  was  investigated,  as  will 
be  noted  below. 


EXPERIMENTAL  RESULTS 

Before  proceeding  to  the  accounts  of  the  results  of  the  experi- 
ments it  appears  desirable  to  present  a  series  of  figures  which 
describe  numerically  the  brains  of  the  animals  on  which  the 
experiments  were  performed.  These  figures  are  given  for  the 
reason  that  the  areal  variations  of  the  stimulable  zones  might 
have  correlations  with  the  brain  sizes  or  brain  weights.  Because 
of  this  there  are  given  various  linear  measurements  and  diameters 
since  the  selection  and  use  of  one  measure  for  possible  correla- 
tion might  be  considered  to  be  too  arbitrary.  The  measurements 
were  made  approximately  one  year  subsequent  to  the  performance 
of  the  tests,  after  the  brains  had  been  continuously  in  formalin 
(10  per  cent).  All  measures  were  taken  in  the  nearest  half 
millimeter  or  nearest  half  gram. 

The  diameter  measurements  which  are  recorded  were  made 
with  slide  calipers.  The  total  length  was  taken  as  the  longest 
diameter  between  the  frontal  and  occipital  poles.  The  total 
width  is  the  greatest  side  to  side  diameter.  This  is  sometimes 
found  caudad  to  the  position  of  a  plane  from  the  upper  portion 
of  the  central  fissure  perpendicular  to  the  longitudinal  sulcus. 
Since  the  measurements  just  noted  can  give  only  a  general  view 
of  the  brain  as  a  whole  it  was  also  thought  best  to  take  data 
referring  to  the  frontal  (anterior  to  the  central  fissure)  portions 
of  the  brain.  The  frontal  width  was  taken  on  each  side  from 
the  longitudinal  sulcus  to  the  side  of  the  bzrain,  on  a  plane 
passing  through  the  central  fissure  approximately  one-half  of 
the  distance  from  the  longitudinal  sulcus  to  the  end  of  the  central 
fissure  near  the  fissure  of  Sylvius.  The  frontal  length  is  also 
given  for  the  two  sides  separately.  It  is  the  distance  between 
the  anterior  tip  of  the  brain  and  a  plane  passing  through  the 
brain  and  beginning  at  the  origin  of  the  central  fissure  near  the 
longitudinal  sulcus  and  perpendicular  to  the  latter.  The  length 
of  the  central  fissure  could  not  be  measured  as  accurately  as  the 


VARIATIONS  IN  DISTRIBUTION  OF  MOTOR  CENTERS        93 

lengths  already  mentioned.  A  narrow  pliable  but  non-stretchable 
tape  was  laid  along  the  fissure  beginning  at  either  its  superior  or 
inferior  end  and  passing  over  its  various  curves  as  well  as 
possible.  The  weights  of  the  brain  were  determined  to  the  nearest 
half  gram,  the  weights  being  of  the  cerebrum  alone.  The 
medulla,  cerebellum  and  spinal  cords  'had  not  been  preserved. 
Three  measurements  of  each  kind  were  made;  the  results  were 
averaged  and  in  the  table  the  averages  are  noted  to  the  nearest 
half  millimeter  or  half  gram. 

The  brain  of  monkey  4  was  slightly  flattened  on  the  left  side 
in  its  superior-inferior  diameter.  This  was  probably  due  to  the 
usual  cause,  lack  of  sufficient  protection  from  the  bottom  of  the 
containing  vessel.  At  the  same  time  this  flattening  may  have 
been  accompanied  by  an  elongation  or  a  broadening  of  that  side. 
Since,  however,  the  two  sides  gave  approximately  (within  0.5 
mm.)  the  same  fronto-occipital  measurements  it  does  not  appear 
likely  that  there  has  been  much  variation  in  this  particular.  On 
the  other  hand,  the  variations  in  width  are  found  to  differ  for 
the  two  hemispheres  of  other  animals  and  it  is  impossible  to  say 
with  surety  that  the  superior-inferior  flattening  was  the  cause  of 
greater  width  of  the  left  hemisphere  of  this  animal.  .When  the 
brain  of  monkey  5  was  removed  from  the  skull  both  occipital 
lobes  were  accidentally  cut  and  in  the  process  of  hardening  some 


FIGURE  i.  Representing  the  methods  of  making  the  linear  measurements 
which  are  given  in  the  text  and  in  Table  I :  a,  total  length ;  b,  total  width ; 
c,  frontal  kngth;  d,  frontal  width;  e,  length  of  central  fissure. 


94 


SHEPHERD  IVORY  FRANZ 


separation  of  these  parts  from  the  remainder  of  the  brain  oc- 
curred. When  the  brains  were  measured  these  parts  were  approx- 
imated to  the  remainder  of  the  brain  and  the  measures  made.  If 
there  is  an  inaccuracy  in  the  measurements  of  this  brain  because 
of  this  such  inaccuracy  relates  probably  solely  to  the  total  length. 
The  measurements  which  have  just  been  described  are  given  in 
Table  I  and  the  methods  of  taking  the  linear  measures  are  illus- 
trated in  Figure  i. 

TABLE  I.  Measurements  of  Monkey  Brains.  All  linear  measurements  are 
given  in  millimeters ;  those  of  mass  in  grams.  The  brains  of  monkeys  4  and  5 
were  somewhat  distorted  when  measured,  and  the  linear  measurements  may 
differ  slightly  from  those  given  here  (see  text,  page  93,  for  explanation). 


Monkeys 

Total 
Lengths 

Total 
Widths 

Frontal  Widths 

Frontal 
Lengths 

Lengths  of 
Central  Fissure 

Weights 

R 

L 

R 

L 

R 

L 

I 

2 

3 
4 
5 

70.0 
67.5 
74.0 
67.5 
66.0 

52.0 
48.5 

54-5 
50.0 
51.0 

21.5 

23-5 
24.5 

21.0 
24.O 

24.0 
24.0 
26.O 
23.0 
22.O 

34-0 

32.5 
41.0 

35-o 
36.5 

36.0 
37-0 
39-5 
36-5 
35-o 

32.0 
33-0 
36.0 

33-0 
29.0 

33-o 
33-o 
37-o 
28.0 

3°-5 

69.0 
65.0 
81.3 

I9'5 
60.5 

The  measurements  show  that  monkey  3  had  the  longest,  the 
widest,  and  the  heaviest  brain  and,  whether  measured  by  the 
product  of  frontal  width  x  -frontal  length,  or  by  frontal  length 
x  length  of  the  central  fissure,  which  products  may  reasonably 
be  supposed  to  give  an  indication  of  the  extents  of  the  frontal 
areas,  also  the  largest  frontal  area.  This,  as  will  be  pointed  out  in 
a  subsequent  section  of  this  report,  is  of  special  interest  in  con- 
nection with  the  extent  of  the  stimulable  areas.  In  respect  to 
the  similar  measures  of  the  brain  of  monkey  I  it  will  be  noted 
that  the  right  hemisphere  of  this  animal  was  found  to  be  among 
the  smallest,  although  the  brain  weight  and  total  length  are 
greater  than  those  of  monkeys  2,  4  and  5.  At  the  same  time  it 
will  be  noted  that  the  brain  weight  of  monkey  2  is  the  median, 
that  the  total  width  is  the  least,  and  that  the  products  of  frontal 
width  x  frontal  length  and  frontal  length  x  length  of  central 
fissure  are  not  much  greater  than  those  of  the  brain  of  monkey  i. 


I.     EXTENTS  OF  CEREBRAL  MOTOR  AREAS  FOR  THE 
ARM  AND  LEG  SEGMENTS 

Monkey  4  died  shortly  after  the  stimulation  experiment  had 
been  begun  on  the  left  hemisphere.  Death  appeared  to  be  due 
to  an  excessive  hemorrhage  from  the  longitudinal  sinus  which 
had  not  been  suitably  ligated.  The  results  of  the  experiments 
on  this  animal  can,  therefore,  be  given  for  only  one  hemisphere 
and  in  this  case  the  comparison  of  the  two  hemispheres  is  im- 
possible. A  general  view  of  the  results  on  all  animals  is  given 
in  Figure  2.  As  has  already  been  mentioned,  the  points  of  stimu- 
lation have  been  dealt  with  as  if  they  were  areas  corresponding 
with  the  spaces  surrounding  the  stimulation  points.  The  results 
of  the  experiments  on  each  hemisphere  are  shown  separately, 
the  digits  referring  to  the  monkeys,  the  upper  diagrams  showing 
the  results  of  the  experiments  on  the  right  hemispheres,  and  the 
lower  diagrams  showing  those  on  the  left  hemispheres  of  these 
five  animals.  The  areas  for  the  leg  segment  movements  are 
represented  by  horizontal  lines  and  those  for  the  arm  seg- 
ment movements  by  vertical  lines.  The  heavy  horizontal  lines 
represent  the  respective  longitudinal  sulci,  other  heavy  lines 
represent  the  principal  fissures  (that  of  Rolando,  or  the  central 
fissure,  being  very  plain)  and  the  three  parallel  lines  indicate  the 
locations  of  the  principal  blood  vessels. 

It  will  be  first  noted  that  the  shapes  of  the  areas  differ  con- 
siderably. In  some  cases  the  areas  appear  to  run  practically  paral- 
lel to  the  central  fissures  (iR,  iL,  3R,  3L).5  In  other  cases  the 
form  of  the  areas  is  irregular,  broader  at  the  top,  or  near  the 
longitudinal  sulcus,  i.e.,  near  the  upper  portion  of  the  Rolandic 
fissure,  and  narrower  below.  It  is  also  to  be  noted  that  in  some 
cases,  and  in  all  hemispheres  in  certain  locations,  the  areas  are 
solid,  while  in  others  there  are  zones  in  which  no  leg  or  arm 
reactions  were  obtained.  In  some  of  these  cases  the  points  were 

5  These  figures  and  subsequent  ones  mean  monkey  i  right  hemisphere, 
monkey  i  left  hemisphere,  etc. 


96  SHEPHERD  IVORY  FRANZ 

apparently  "silent"  since  no  movement  of  the  arm,  trunk,  tail 
or  head  segments  followed  stimulation,  but  in  a  few  cases  on 
the  other  hand  the  stimulation  of  these  areas  gave  movements  of 
the  head  or  of  a  segment  other  than  those  which  are  dealt  with 
here  (i.e.,  arm  and  leg).  These  anomalous  results  will  be  dis- 
cussed in  a  later  section  of  this  article  (see  p.  134  ff.). 

A  third  difference  which  is  obvious  is  that  the  overlapping  of 
arm  and  leg  areas  is  irregular.  In  the  brain  of  monkey  i  none 
of  this  overlapping  was  discovered,  very  little  was  found  in  the 
brains  of  monkeys  3  and  4  and  more  was  found  in  the  brain  of 
monkey  5,  and  a  considerable  degree  of  overlapping  was  dis- 
covered in  the  brain  of  monkey  2.  These  overlappings  will  also 
be  dealt  with  in  a  subsequent  section  (see  p.  128  ff.). 

A  further  difference,  which  may  however  be  only  casual,  is 
that  the  dividing  line  between  the  leg  and  arm  areas  is  at  times 
well  marked  by  fissures  or  by  blood  vessels  and  in  the  brains  of 
other  animals  these  anatomical  landmarks  do  not  appear  to  have 
physiological  differentiating  characters.  Those  hemispheres  in 
which  fissures  and  blood  vessels  mark  off  the  two  areas  under 
consideration  are  iR,  iL,  2L,  3L,  and  5L.  I  do  not  think  that 
the  greater  frequency  of  this  in  the  left  hemispheres,  or  in  fact 
any  of  the  differences  in  this  particular  can,  with  our  present 
knowledge,  be  considered  to  be  of  physiological  significance. 

A  closer  examination  of  the  figures  also  reveals  well  marked 
differences  in  the  totals  of  the  stimulable  areas.  It  is  obvious 
from  inspection  that  the  stimulable  areas  of  2L  is  greater  than 
that  of  all  other  hemispheres  which  have  been  examined.  It  is 
also  apparent  that  in  this  respect  there  are  great  variations, 
hemisphere  4R  shows  the  smallest  area  and  the  remaining  seven 
hemispheres  are  intermediate  between  4R  and  2L.  As  has  al- 
ready been  indicated  (p.  91 )  there  was  an  experimental  error 
in  connection  with  hemisphere  3L  inasmuch  as  there  was  a  possi* 
ble  line  bordering  upon  the  longitudinal  sulcus  which  was  not 
subjected  to  stimulation.  In  this  one  case  had  the  stimuli  been 
given  to  points  in  this  area  it  is  possible  that  the  leg  area  would 
have  been  found  to  extend  correspondingly  in  the  wedge  shape 
upwards  to  the  longitudinal  sulcus. 


SHEPHERD  IVORY  FRANZ 


More  careful  measurements  of  these  areas  reveal  the  differ- 
•ences  which  are  observable  on  inspection.  The  figures  represent- 
ing these  measurements  are  given  in  Table  II.  The  measure- 
ments were  made  from  the  magnified  (10  diameters)  charts 

TABLE  II.  Measurements  of  Areas  of  Stimulation  of  Monkey  Brains. 
Figures  marked  with  an  asterisk  are  probably  too  low.  For  explanation  of 
this  see  the  text,  page  91. 


Monkeys 

Hemispheres 

Areas  in  square  millimeters 

Per  cent 
Leg 

Leg 

Arm 

Totals 

Overlappings 

Net 

Arm 
40 

121 

I 

R 

L 

33 
86 

82 
71 

"5 
157 

0 

O 

"5 
157 

2 

R 
L 

61 
124 

1  06 
125 

167 
249 

IS 
34 

152 

£15 

58 

99 

3 

R 

L 

25 

57* 

103 
80 

128 
137* 

4 
5 

124 
132* 

24 
71* 

4 

R 

Si 

34 

85 

7 

78 

150 

5 

R 
L 

79 
78 

83 
77 

162 

'55 

23 
ii 

139 

144 

96 

IOI 

which  have  been  described,  by  the  aid  of  a  transparent  die 
divided  to  show  actual  square  millimeters  (in  the  magnified 
form  square  centimeters,  of  course)  which  was  placed  over 
the  areas.  Each  full  square  covering  the  stimulable  zone 
was  counted  as  one  and  each  part  square  as  one-half,  the 
latter  on  the  supposition  that  the  areas  larger  than  one-half 
would  counterbalance  those  smaller  than  one-half.  The 
results  of  this  comparatively  rough  method  were  compared  in  one 
case  with  the  similar  finer  method  of  using  a  die  with  spaces 
representing  half-millimeter  squares,  and  since  the  variations 
did  not  greatly  exceed  one  per-  cent  the  original  measurements 
were  considered  to  be  sufficiently  accurate.  The  method  of 
measurement  is  also  obviously  exact  enough  in  view  of  the 
methods  which  were  employed  for  the  limitation  of  the  areas 
involved,  and  especially  in  view  of  the  magnified  representations 
of  the  areas  which  were  measured.  It  should  be  remarked, 
however,  that  a  turning  of  the  die  through  an  angle  of  30  degrees 
varied  the  measures  by  as  much  as  6  per  cent  but  even  with  this 
variation  the  figures  appear  to  be  sufficiently  exact  as  they  stand. 


VARIATIONS  IN  DISTRIBUTION  OF  MOTOR  CENTERS        99 

In  connection  with  the  table  mention  may  be  made  again  of  the 
fact  that  in  hemisphere  3L  an  area  lying  next  to  the  longitudinal 
sulcus  was  not  stimulated.  The  figures  in  columns  3,  5,  7  and  8 
referring  to  this  hemisphere  are,  possibly,  too  low. 

At  the  present  moment  attention  should  be  directed  solely  to 
column  7  of  this  table  in  which  are  given  the  net  totals  of  the 
areas  which  were  found  to  be  stimulable.  These  figures  show  that 
the  stimulable  area  of  4R  is  the  smallest,  that  of  2L  the  greatest. 
In  terms  of  percentage,  using  the  lowest  figure  as  100,  we  find 
that  the  other  hemispheres  take  the  following  order  and  show 
the  following  percents :  iR  (149);  3R  (159);  3L  (169);  5R 
(178)  ;  2R  (195)  ;  iL  (201)  ;  2L  (276).  It  will  thus  be  seen 
that  the  greatest  area  (2L)  is  almost  three  times  as  large  as 
that  of  4R,  and  that  the  other  seven  hemispheres  vary  from 
50  per  cent  to  100  per  cent  greater  than  the  smallest. 

These  data  show  that  not  only  are  there  marked  variations 
in  different  animals,  but  also  that  the  variations  of  the  two 
hemispheres  of  the  same  animal  are  sometimes  great  with  re- 
spect to  the  stimulable  (motor)  areas.  These  differences  of 
the  two  hemispheres  of  the  same  animal  are  worthy  of  some 
notice.  In  all  four  monkeys  of  which  both  hemispheres  were 
investigated  and  measured  it  will  be  noted  that  the  stimulable 
areas  on  the  left  surpass  those  on  the  right.  In  the  case  of 
monkey  5  this  left-sided  preponderance  is  not  great,  only  3.6 
per  cent  and  therefore  within  the  error  of  measurement,  and 
similarly  in  the  case  of  monkey  3,  only  6.5  per  cent,  close  to 
the  error  of  measurement.  In  the  latter  case,  however,  there 
should  be  recalled  the  suggestion  (see  p.  96)  of  the  possi- 
bility that  the  leg  area  should  be  considered  to  be  larger  than 
it  is  sketched.  With  respect  to  monkeys  i  and  2  the  pre- 
ponderance of  the  left  side  is  great,  36  and  41  per  cent  re- 
spectively. In  the  next  section  a  further  analysis  of  these  figures 
will  be  made  to  determine  the  relative  areas  for  the  arm  and 
leg  movements  separately. 

To  what  factors  these  differences  correspond  is  at  present 
unknown.  Certain  of  the  general  objections  to  considering  them 
typical  of  the  motor  cortex  have  already  been  discussed 


100  SHEPHERD  IVORY  FRANZ 

(p.  89  ff.)  and  the  general  relation  of  the  differences  to  our 
conception  of  cerebral  activities  will  be  considered  in  the  section 
devoted  to  the  theoretical  discussion  (p.  140  ff.).  At  this  point, 
however,  it  may  be  well  to  show  the  general  lack  of  correspond- 
ence or  correlation  with  the  measurements  of  the  brain  which 
have  been  recorded  in  Table  I  (p.  94).  Monkey  3,  with 
the  greatest  brain  weight,  greatest  length  of  hemisphere,  and 
greatest  width  of  the  cerebrum  did  not  have  the  most  extensive 
motor  area  as  determined  by  the  methods  which  were  used  in 
the  present  work,  while  monkey  2,  with  one  of  the  smallest 
brains  in  the  series  showed  the  most  extensive  excitable  areas. 
If  the  measures  which  have  been  used  (frontal  length  x  frontal 
width,  and  frontal  length  x  length  of  central  fissure}  are  at  all 
typical  of  the  amount  of  the  cortex  of  the  frontal  areas,  it  is 
plain  that  there  is  no  direct  relation  between  the  extent  of  the 
motor  or  stimulable  cortex  and  the  total  amount  of  the  cortex 
anterior  to  the  central  fissure.  Such  a  conclusion  becomes  more 
evident  if  we  deal  with  the  data  regarding  the  other  hemispheres. 
The  only  apparent  fact  which  indicates  a  possible  relation  be- 
tween brain  size  (or  amount  of  frontal  cortex)  and  the  extents 
of  the  motor  area  is  that  there  is  a  closer  correspondence  be- 
tween the  relative  sizes  of  the  motor  areas  and  the  total  areas 
of  the  frontal  lobes  in  the  two  hemispheres  of  the  same  animal. 
Thus,  it  might  be  concluded  that  the  preponderance  of  the  motor 
area  of  the  left  hemisphere  is  an  indication  of  and  bears  a 
possible  correlation  with  the  (in  general)  larger  hemisphere 
measurements  on  that  side.  Monkey  i  whose  brain  showed 
greater  width,  greater  frontal  length,  and  greater  length  of 
the  central  fissure  on  the  left  showed  also  a  considerable  su- 
periority in  the  extent  of  the  motor  area  on  that  side.  The 
brain  of  monkey  2,  in  which  a  similar  superiority  in  size  was 
apparent,  except  for  fissure  length,  also  showed  a  larger  area 
on  the  left  side.  The  brain  of  monkey  3,  in  which  there  was 
a  greater  width  and  a  greater  fissure  length  on  the  left  while 
the  left  frontal  length  was  smaller  than  that  on  the  right  showed 
little  difference  in  the  relative  sizes  of  the  two  motor  areas. 
In  a  similar  manner  the  brain  of  monkey  5  showed  variations 


VARIATIONS  IN  DISTRIBUTION  OF  MOTOR  CENTERS      101 

in  measurements,  some  of  the  right  hemisphere  being  greater 
than  those  of  the  left  and  others  of  the  left  being  greater  than 
the  corresponding  measures  of  the  right  hemisphere.  The 
cerebral  measures  which  may  be  concluded  to  be  approximately 
balancing  for  the  two  sides  correspond  therefore  with  the  almost 
equal  distribution  of  the  motor  areas.  A  disturbing  element  to 
such  a  conclusion  is  due  to  the  unfortunate  failure  to  complete 
the  series  of  experiments  on  the  left  hemisphere  of  monkey  3. 
While  there  is  an  equal  reason  for  believing  that  if  the  stimuli 
had  been  given  in  this  area  there  would  and  there  would  not 
have  been  any  great  change  in  the  sum  totals  of  the  areas, 
at  least  the  case  must  tentatively  be  thrown  out  of  consideration. 
Another  fact  which  is  opposed  to  the  conclusion  of  such  a 
definite  relation  is  found  in  the  lack  of  correspondence  between 
the  relations  of  the  hemisphere  measurements  and  the  relations 
of  the  extents  of  the  excitable  zones.  Those  measurements  which 
have  been  taken  to  represent  the  areas  of  the  frontal  lobes 
(frontal  length  x  length  of  the  central  fissure,  and  frontal  length 
x  frontal  width}  do  not  have  the  same  or  nearly  the  same  pro- 
portions that  the  total  motor  areas  of  the  two  hemispheres  of 
the  same  animal  bear.  Thus  our  relative  measures  for  the 
brains  R/L  are  as  follows :  frontal  length  x  -frontal  width, 
1=0.85,  2  =  0.86;  3  —  1.01;  5=10.99.  To  compare  with 
these  figures  we  have  the  similarly  calculated  relations  of  the 
two  motor  areas  of  the  same  brains  as  follows:  1=0.73; 
2  =  0.71;  3  =  0.94;  5—0.97.  The  absolute  figures  do  not 
show  a  close  correspondence  but  it  must  be  admitted  that  the 
measures  are  grossly  inaccurate  as  representing  the  area  of  the 
anterior  parts  of  the  cortex  of  the  cerebrum.  If  we  consider  the 
relative  figures  there  appears  to  be  a  closer  correlation  inasmuch 
as  the  relatively  smaller  motor  areas  on  the  right  (monkeys  i 
and  2)  may  be  compared  (not  directly,  however)  with  the 
smaller  cortical  zones  on  that  side.  At  the  same  time  the  nearly 
equal  motor  areas  (monkeys  3  and  5)  are  to  be  compared  with 
the  nearly  equal  cortical  zones  (or  with  the  preponderating  right 
hemisphere  of  monkey  5  as  indicated  by  frontal  length  x  -frontal 


102  SHEPHERD  IVORY  FRANZ 

width).     We  can  conclude  with  certainty  that  if  a  relation  in 
this  respect  exists  it  is  neither  simple  nor  direct. 

Summary:  The  motor  areas  for  the  leg  and  arm  segments 
differ  in  size  in  the  brains  of  different  animals,  and  in  the  two 
hemispheres  of  the  same  animal.  These  differences  are  not  ac- 
counted for  by  the  size  variations  of  the  hemisphere  of  the 
the  different  animals  although  there  is  some  indication  of  a 
possibility  of  correlation  of  the  sizes  of  the  frontal  lobes  and 
the  extents  of  the  motor  areas  of  the  two  hemispheres  of  the 
same  animal. 


II.     RELATIVE  DISTRIBUTIONS  OF  AREAS  FOR  LEG 
AND  ARM  MOVEMENTS 

Figure  2  and  Table  II  also  contain  data  regarding  the  absolute 
and  relative  extents  of  the  respective  areas  for  the  movements 
of  the  posterior  and  the  anterior  limbs.  Examination  of  the 
parts  of  the  figure  and  of  columns  3,  4,  7,  and  8  of  the  table 
reveal  extensive  differences.  These  differences  are  (i)  varying 
amounts  of  cerebral  areas  in  different  animals  for  the  leg  and 
for  the  arm  movements,  (2)  varying  amounts  of  cerebral  areas 
in  the  two  hemispheres  of  the  same  animal  for  the  leg  and  for 
the  arm  movements,  (3)  variations  in  the  overlapping  or  mixing 
of  the  leg  and  arm  areas,  which  matter  will  be  reserved  for 
discussion  in  a  subsequent  section,  and  (4)  variations  in  the 
spatial  proportions  of  these  two  areas  in  the  same  hemisphere. 

The  individual  variations  in  extents  of  these  areas  should  first 
be  noted.  The  smallest  leg  area  was  found  in  3R,  the  largest 
in  2L.  The  smallest  arm  area  was  found  in  4R,  the  largest  in 
2L.  The  largest  leg  and  arm  areas  were  found  in  the  hemisphere 
with  the  largest  total  area,  which  as  noted  above  was  by  no 
means  the  largest  brain.  The  smallest  leg  area  was  not  found 
in  the  hemisphere  with  the  smallest  total  stimulable  zone,  but 
the  smallest  arm  area  was  found  in  the  hemisphere  with  the 
smallest  net  total  stimulable  area.  The  intermediate  sized  total 
areas  more  closely  correspond  with  the  order  of  magnitude 
of  the  leg  areas  than  with  those  of  the  arm  areas.  Thus  we 
find  the  order  of  magnitude  of  the  net  totals  of  the  stimulable 
zones  (combined  arm  and  leg  areas)  are  4R,  iR,  3R,  3L,  5R,  5L, 
2R,  iL,  and  2L;  the  order  for  the  leg  areas  is  3R,  iR,  4R,  3L, 
2R,  5L,  5R,  iL,  and  2L;  and  the  order  for  the  arm  areas  is 
4R,  iL,  5L,  3L,  iR,  5R,  3R,  2R,  and  2L.  The  serial  orders 
indicate  a  rough  correlation  between  the  individual  arm  and  leg 
and  the  net  total  areas,  with  a  greater  correspondence  of  net 
total  with  leg  areas.  When,  however,  the  percentage  relations 
of  the  individual  areas  are  considered  it  is  to  be  noted  that  the 
only  close  correspondence  is  in  the  hemispheres  in  which  the 
leg  and  arm  areas  are  nearly  equal  in  size.  Thus  we  find  in 


104  SHEPHERD  IVORY  FRANZ 

general  a  fairly  close  correspondence  in  the  cases  of  2L,  5R 
and  5L,  but  in  these  cases  with  the  leg  and  the  arm  areas 
each  about  one-half  of  the  total,  a  direct  and  proportional  corre- 
spondence is  obviously  the  only  possible  relation  that  can  exist. 

When  we  examine  the  table  we  find  that  in  only  monkey 
5  are  the  totals  of  the  areas  for  the  leg  and  for  the  arm  closely 
similar  for  the  two  hemispheres.  The  almost  exact  correspond- 
ence of  areal  distribution  for  the  leg  movements  is  noteworthy 
and  the  differences  in  the  sizes  of  arm  areas  in  this  animal  are 
not  great,  perhaps  not  much  greater  than  the  errors  of  record- 
ing and  of  calculation.  The  only  other  close  correspondence  is  for 
the  arm  areas  of  monkey  I,  but  in  this  case  the  variation  is 
approximately  fifteen  per  cent.  In  the  other  five  cases  (hemi- 
spheres) the  differences  are  greater,  the  variations  ranging  from 
1 8  to  over  100  per  cent.  In  the  left  hemispheres  of  monkeys 
i,  2  and  3,  the  leg  areas  are  larger  than  those  of  the  right. 
In  the  left  hemisphere  of  monkey  2  the  arm  area  is  the  larger, 
while  the  arm  area  is  larger  in  the  right  hemisphere  of  monkey  3. 
It  will  be  noticed,  therefore,  that  the  left  motor  areas  for  the 
leg  are  in  general  considerably  larger  than  those  on  the  right, 
while  a  reverse  condition  holds  for  the  arms  areas  of  the  two 
hemispheres  with  the  exception  of  monkey  2  and  also  with  the 
exception  that  the  differences  are  not  as  great. 

If  other  data  were  not  at  hand  such  variations  might  reason- 
ably be  thought  to  bear  a  possible  relation  to  the  sizes  of  the 
hemispheres,  but  an  examination  of  the  figures  in  Table  I  and 
comparisons  with  those  of  Table  II  show  that  no  such  relation 
exists. 

Coupled  with  the  individual  and  the  hemisphere  variations  of 
these  areas  we  may  also  consider  the  relations  to  each  other 
of  the  leg  and  arm  areas  of  the  same  hemisphere.  In  this 
comparison  we  note  even  greater  deviations  than  have  already 
been  discussed.  The  quotients  of  leg  area  divided  by  arm  area 
for  the  individual  hemispheres  are  shown  in  column  8  of  Table 
II.  The  smallest  is  that  of  3R,  the  largest  is  that  of  4R.  In 
three  hemispheres  (2L,  5R,  and  5L)  the  quotients  show  the 
two  areas  to  be  about  equal,  in  four  hemispheres  the  quotients 
show  considerable  spatial  superiority  of  the  arm  area  (iR,  2R, 


VARIATIONS  IN  DISTRIBUTION  OF  MOTOR  CENTERS      105 

3R,  and  3L),  and  in  two  hemispheres  a  corresponding  superiority 
of  the  leg  area  (iL  and  4R).  Although  the  importance  of  the 
observation  is  not  apparent  it  is  interesting  to  find  that  in  the 
four  cases  in  which  figures  for  both  hemispheres  are  available 
the  relative  superiority  of  the  arm  area  is  more  noticeable  on 
the  right.  It  will  be  noted  also  that  in  three  of  these  cases 
(monkeys  i,  2,  and  3)  this  relative  superiority  is  large  and, 
in  the  other  case,  although  small,  it  is  indicated  by  a  difference 
of  at  least  5  per  cent. 

An  explanation  of  these  differences  cannot  be  given  at  the 
present  time.  I  greatly  regret  that  extended  observations  of  the 
behavior  of  the  animals  were  not  made  previous  to  the  experi- 
ments, for,  merely  to  speculate,  the  suggestion  occurs  that  these 
cortical  variations  may  have  some  relation  to  the  normal  activities 
of  the  individual  animals.  A  few  observations  regarding  the  use 
of  the  right  and  left  hands  of  some  of  the  animals  were  made 
but  the  data  are  so  few  that  they  give  no  clue  to  a  possible  rela- 
tion between  the  extensive  or  limited  arm  areas  in  one  hemisphere 
and  the  use  of  the  right  or  left  hand  or  arm.  It  is  for  our 
present  purposes  unfortunate  that  even  these  inadequate  tests 
were  not  continued  a  sufficient  length  of  time  with  one  animal 
to  make  certain  any  preference  in  the  employment  of  the  hands. 
The  suggestion  of  a  possible  correlation  of  the  areal  differences 
and  the  differences  in  behavior  is  directly  in  line  with  previous 
conceptions  of  cerebral  function,  especially  those  regarding  the 
relations  of  the  associational  areas  to  occupations  and  habits 
of  thought.  An  extended  series  of  observations  of  habits,  gen- 
eral activity,  etc.,  of  many  animals  is  needed  along  with  corre- 
sponding observations  of  the  stimulable  areas  before  the  truth 
of  such  a  supposition  can  be  determined. 

Summary :  The  areas  for  the  arm  and  for  the  leg  differ  to 
a  considerable  extent  in  different  animals,  and  to  an  equal 
degree  in  the  two  hemispheres  of  the  same  animal.  The  leg 
areas  are  sometimes  larger  than,  more  frequently  smaller  than, 
and  at  times  approximately  equal  to  the  corresponding  arm  areas. 
In  the  two  hemispheres  of  the  same  animal  the  quotients  of 
leg  area  divided  by  arm  area  are  not  even  approximately  equal, 
the  proportions  varying  by  as  much  as  one  to  three. 


Ill    DISTRIBUTIONS  OF  THE  AREAS  FOR  THE  INDI- 
VIDUAL SEGMENTS 

Up  to  this  point  we  have  dealt  with  the  areas  for  the  arm 
and  leg  as  if  these  were  the  main  anatomical  (and  physiological) 
elements  which  were  to  be  considered.  It  is  obvious,  however, 
that  the  individual  segments  of  these  larger  anatomical  units 
are  worthy  of  more  and  closer  study.  We  shall  also  find  that 
the  variations  which  have  already  been  shown  to  exist  are  not 
only  paralleled,  but  in  a  number  of  cases  they  are  exceeded  in 
amount,  by  the  variations  in  relative  sizes  of  the  areas  for  the 
smaller  segments.  At  the  present  time  the  analysis  and  com- 
parison of  the  types  of  the  movements  have  not  been  attempted, 
nor  will  the  separate  finger  or  toe  movements  be  dealt  with 
individually.  For  the  present  study  I  have  made  'eight  groups 
of  movements  as  follows:  thigh,  which  includes  all  mass  move- 
ments of  the  leg  on  the  trunk,  such  movements  being  of  the 
upper  part  of  the  leg;  leg,  those  movements  at  the  knee;  foot, 
those  movements  at  the  ankle ;  toes,  movements  of  these  elements 
taken  collectively  and  not  at  the  present  time  differentiating  the 
movements  of  individual  toes ;  shoulder,  those  movements  of  the 
upper  arm  in  relation  to  the  remainder  of  the  body;  forearm, 
movements  of  the  elbow;  hand,  movements  of  this  organ  at 
the  wrist;  and  fingers,  movements  of  these  parts,  also  collectively 
without  differentiating  the  movements  of  the  individual  fingers 
or  the  thumb.  At  the  same  time  I  have  for  the  present  disre- 
garded the  characters  of  the  movements,  (i)  whether  they  be 
flexions,  or  extensions,  or  rotations,  or  (2)  dealing  with  the 
movements  as  they  appear  in  their  complexity  as  behavior 
phenomena,  whether  they  be  of  a  thrusting,  or  of  a  grasping, 
or  of  a  propulsive,  or  a  reaching,  or  of  any  other  complex  nature. 
In  this  section,  therefore,  I  deal  with  the  movements  of 
anatomical  segments  and  not  with  the  movement  characters. 

Thigh. — The  distributions  of  the  areas  the  .stimulation  of 
which  resulted  in  movements  of  the  thigh  are  shown  in  the 
diagrams  of  Figure  3.  The  relative  areal  variations  are  here 


io8 


SHEPHERD  IVORY  FRANZ 


observed  to  be  much  greater  than  those  for  the  combined  leg 
segments  as  shown  in  Figure  2,  or  than  those  for  the  arm 
segment  as  shown  in  the  same  figure.  In  two  hemispheres, 
iR  and  3R,  these  areas  do  not  touch  the  longitudinal  sulcus, 
while  in  all  other  cases  (omitting  3L  which  as  has  already  been 
mentioned  was  not  sufficiently  investigated  in  this  respect)  the 
thigh  areas  border  upon  this  great  fissure.  It  is  also  to  be 
noted  that  with  the  exception  of  the  two  hemispheres  of  monkey 
i,  and  it  may  be  said  that  the  left  hemisphere  of  this  animal 
is  a  rather  doubtful  case  in  this  particular,  all  of  the  thigh  areas 
border  upon  the  central  fissure.  In  some  of  the  cases  the  loca- 
tions are  suggestive  of  outcroppings  from  the  central  fissure, 
and  of  extensions  of  similarly  functioning  cortical  zones  lying 
within  that  fissure.  In  the  same  way  we  may  consider  the  areas 
bordering  upon  the  longitudinal  sulcus  although  this  appears 
a  less  probable  explanation,  except  in  the  case  of  monkey  5. 
In  all  other  animals  there  is  a  constriction  of  the  area  towards 
the  longitudinal  sulcus,  the  greater  extents  being  on  the  convexity 
away  from  that  zone. 

The  extensive  variations  of  the  area  are  shown  in  the  ac- 
companying Table  III  which  gives  numerical  expression  to  the 
diagrams.  Reference  to  this  Table  will  be  sufficient  without 

TABLE  III.  Measurements  of  the  extents  of  the  thigh  areas.  The  figure 
marked  with  an  asterisk  is  probably  too  low  (see  page  91)  and  the  corre- 
sponding percentage  R/L  too  high. 


Monkeys 

I 

2 

3 

4 

5 

Hemispheres 

R        L 

R        L 

R        L 

R 

R        L 

Areas  in  square 
mm. 

4-5      30.0 

47.5     107.0 

17.5    *i6.o 

25.0 

53-0      46.0 

Percentages 
R/L 

15 

44 

109 

— 

"5 

Percentage  re- 
lations of  av- 
erages, Mon- 
key 2  =  IOO. 

22 

IOO 

22 

32 

65 

VARIATIONS  IN  DISTRIBUTION  OF  MOTOR  CENTERS      109 

any  textural  discussion,  since  the  data  are  self-explanatory. 
The  attention  of  the  reader  is  particularly  directed  to  the  two 
final  lines  in  which  are  noted  (i)  the  proportions  of  the  areas 
in  the  two  hemispheres  of  the  same  animals,  and  also  (2)  the 
percentage  relations  of  the  average  extents  of  these  areas  in 
the  five  animals,  using  the  largest  average  as  unity. 

In  addition  to  the  variations  in  the  absolute  and  relative  sizes 
of  these  areas  the  distribution  of  the  stimulable  zones  is  worthy 
of  remark.  With  the  exception  of  iR,  in  which  there  appears 
only  a  small  superficial  area,  all  brains  show  a  wide-spread 
distribution.  In  no  case  (except  iR)  is  the  area  solid,  but  the 
points  are  frequently  separated  by  the  cerebral  zones  for  other 
movements  or  by  the  so-called  silent  or  non-stimulable  areas. 
In  some  cases  this  separation,  which  will  also  be  found  illus- 
trated in  some  of  the  later  diagrams,  is  noteworthy  since  the 
separated  areas  are  within  the  zones  governing  the  movements 
of  the  arm  segments  and  also  because  they  are  at  such  relatively 
great  distances  from  the  main  masses  of  the  cortex  which  may 
appropriately  be  called  the  primary  areas.  In  iL  and  in  2L 
these  separations  are  especially  great. 

Leg. — Similar  variations  in  the  extents  and  in  the  distribution 
of  the  areas  governing  the  movements  of  the  lower  part  of  the 
leg  are  to  be  noted  *by  inspection  of  the  diagrams  of  Figure  4 
and  the  data  in  Table  IV.  As  compared  with  the  areas  for 
the  thigh  movements  some  hemispheres  show  a  greater  leg  area 
(hemispheres  iR,  2R,  3R,  3L  and  5R)  while  others  (iL,  2L 
and  4R)  show  a  lesser  leg  area.  The  general  distribution  of 
the  areas  does  not  differ  markedly  for  these  two  segments,  al- 
though the  forms  of  the  areas  are  not  nearly  the  same.  Both 
thigh  areas  and  leg  areas  are  located  at  the  upper  portion  of 
the  fissure  of  Rolando  with  perhaps  a  little  more  extensive  ad- 
vance towards  the  lower  portion  on  the  part  of  the  leg  area. 
Most  of  the  points  in  both  areas,  as  can  be  seen  by  placing 
the  two  figures  together,  are  duplicates,  indicating  that  the  move- 
ments are  combined  movements  of  thigh  and  leg.  In  a  few 
cases,  especially  in  hemispheres  iR  and  3L,  the  leg  movements 
were  not  combined  with  movements  at  the  thigh. 


no 


SHEPHERD  IVORY  FRANZ 


The  percentage  relations  of  the  two  hemispheres  of  the  same 
animal  and  those  of  the  average  areas  in  the  five  animals  are 
very  great.  It  will  be  noticed  that  the  relation  R/L  varies  from 

TABLE  IV.  Measurements  of  the  extents  of  leg  areas.  The  figure  marked 
with  an  asterisk  is  probably  too  low  (&ee  page  91)  and  the  corresponding 
percentage  R/L  too  high. 


Monkeys 

I 

2 

3 

4 

5 

Hemispheres 

R        L 

R        L 

R        L 

R 

R        L 

Areas  in  square 
mm. 

25-5      16.5 

56.5     89.0 

20.5    *30.5 

16.0 

60.0      46.5 

Percentages 
R/L 

155 

63 

67 

- 

129 

Percentage  re- 
lations of  av- 
erages, Mon- 
key 2  =  IOO. 

29 

IOO 

35 

22 

74 

63  to  155  per  cent.  The  percentage  relations  of  the  average 
extents  of  the  areas  with  the  extents  of  the  areas  in  monkey 
2  as  unity  are  also  greatly  different,  ranging  from  22  to  74. 
In  no  case  do  these  proportions  correspond  with  the  proportions 
for  the  thigh  areas,  the  nearest  approach  to  correspondence  being 
in  the  case  of  monkey  5.  It  can  be  concluded,  therefore,  that 
neither  the  absolute  nor  the  relative  extents  of  the  areas  for 
the  thigh  and  leg  movements  closely  correspond. 

Foot.* — Variations,  both  for  absolute  and  relative  amounts  of 
the  areas,  similar  to  those  which  have  already  been  described 
for  the  thigh  and  leg  are  also  noticed  for  the  foot  areas.  These 
are  shown  in  the  diagrams  of  Figure  5  and  in  Table  V.  These 
variations  exist  not  only  for  the  different  animals  but  also  for 
the  two  hemispheres  of  the  same  animal.  In  the  latter  cases, 
however,  the  correspondence  is  closer  than  in  the  former.  If 
we  take  the  net  totals  as  standards,  that  is,  if  we  take  the  com- 
bined stimulable  zones  for  the  arm  and  leg  segments  as  standards 
for  each  hemisphere,  we  find  that  the  percentages  of  the  areas 


VARIATIONS  IN  DISTRIBUTION  OF  MOTOR  CENTERS      113 
TABLE  V.    Measurements  of  the  extents  of  foot  areas. 


Monkeys 

I 

2 

3 

4 

5 

Hemispheres 

R        L 

R        L 

R        L 

R 

R        L 

Areas  in  square 
mm. 

9.0      23.0 

34-5      52.5 

13-5      35-0 

24.0 

50.5      42.0 

Percentages 
R/L 

39 

66 

39 

— 

1  20 

Percentage  re- 
lations of  av- 
erages, Mon- 
key 2  =  IOO. 

37 

IOO 

56 

55 

163 

for  the  foot  movements  for  the  two  hemispheres  of  one  animal 
vary  in  two  cases  (monkeys  I  and  3)  and  do  not  vary  greatly 
in  the  other  two  cases  (monkeys  2  and  5).  In  relation  to  the 
standards  which  have  just  been  mentioned  we  find  the  foot 
areas  with  the  following  per  cents:  iR  — 8;  iL=i5; 
2R  =  23;  2L  =  25;  3R=n;  3L  =  27;  4R  =  3I5  5R  =  36; 
5L  =  29.  In  other  words  the  total  extents  of  the  areas  gov- 
erning the  movements  of  the  foot  vary  from  9  per  cent.  (iR) 
to  36  per  cent.  (sR)  of  the  total  areas  for  arm  and  leg.  These 
percentage  data  do  not  correspond  closely  with  the  actual  ex- 
tents of  the  areas,  which  vary  from  9  sq.  mm.  (iR)  to  52.5 
sq.  mm.  (2L).  Nor  do  the  relations  R/L  of  actual  measure- 
ments have  the  same  proportions  and  the  same  serial  order 
as  the  relations  R/L  of  the  percentages  of  the  totals.  This 
failure  of  correspondence  is  due,  of  course,  to  the  varying  degree 
of  overlapping  areas  in  the  different  hemispheres.  The  over- 
lapping of  areas  does  not  entirely  change  the  relative  positions 
of  the  different  hemispheres,  however,  for  hemisphere  iR  still 
remains  the  lowest,  followed  by  3R.  The  position  of  4R  is, 
however,  considerably  changed  in  that  by  this  comparison  it 
shows  a  relatively  large  area  for  foot  movements  whereas  in 
absolute  amounts  it  is  the  fourth  lowest. 

Comparisons  of  the  areal  amounts  for  the  foot  with  those  for 


114  SHEPHERD  IVORY  FRANZ 

the  thigh  and  the  leg  show  many  interesting  relations.  In 
the  brains  of  monkeys  2  and  5  the  foot  areas  are  less  than 
either  the  thigh  or  leg  areas,  although  in  both  hemispheres  of 
monkey  5  the  superiority  of  the  thigh  over  the  foot  is  small 
and,  perhaps,  within  the  error  of  calculation.  In  3R  a  similar 
relation  holds,  although  on  the  left  the  reverse  condition  is 
found.  In  monkey  4  and  in  monkey  i  the  relation  is  not  con- 
stant, the  thigh  area  being  larger  than  the  foot  area  in  iL  and 
less  in  iR  and  in  4R. 

The  figure  illustrating  the  distribution  of  the  foot  areas  in 
the  different  hemispheres  shows  other  interesting  variations.  In 
a  number  of  cases  we  find  that,  unlike  the  corresponding  areas 
for  the  thigh  and  for  the  leg,  there  has  been  a  sort  of  diffusion 
or  scattering  of  the  foot  areas.  The  diagrammatic  representa- 
tion of  the  points  of  stimulation  shows  less  of  a  coalesced 
mass  and  more  individual  patches.  Whether  or  not  this  has 
any  anatomical  or  physiological  significance  cannot  be  deter- 
mined. A  similar  condition  will  later  be  noted  for  some  of  the 
areas  for  the  arm  segments. 

Another  matter  which  may  be  called  to  the  attention  of  the 
reader  is  the  occurrence  of  points  or  areas  for  "pure"  move- 
ments of  the  foot.  A  careful  comparison  of  figures  3,  4,  and 
5  shows  that  there  are  certain  points  in  the  foot  area  which 
have  no  overlapping  of  thigh  and  leg  areas,  and  a  further  com- 
parison with  figure  6  shows  a  similar  state  of  affairs  as  related 
to  toe  movements.  Thus  we  find  in  iR  a  small  area,  at  the 
upper  portion  of  the  fissure  of  Rolando  which  borders  upon  the 
longitudinal  sulcus,  which  is  not  duplicated  in  any  of  the  other 
three  diagrams  for  the  leg  movements  in  this  hemisphere.  Also 
in  iL  there  is  a  similar  zone  at  the  angle  of  the  large  blood 
vessel,  and  a  second  zone  at  the  extreme  upper  portion  of  the 
area.  Similar  zones  are  found  in  four  other  hemispheres;  in 
4R  at  the  extreme  right  upper  portion,  in  5R  a  small  area  in 
the  lower  part  of  the  solid,  in  5L  another  zone  bordering  upon 
and  equidistant  from  the  indicated  extremities  of  the  blood 
vessel  and  towards  the  fissure  of  Rolando,  and  in  3!,  a  zone 
of  this  character  at  the  left  end  of  the  solid  area.  It  will  thus 


VARIATIONS  IN  DISTRIBUTION  OF  MOTOR  CENTERS      115 

be  seen  that  scattered  throughout  the  solid  area  for  the  leg 
there  is  found  an  occasional  zone  for  movements  of  one  portion 
of  the  anatomical  segment  uncomplicated  with  movements  of 
other  segments. 

Toes. — The  relative  positions  of  the  toe  areas  is  noteworthy. 
A  comparison  of  the  diagrams  in  Figure  3,  4,  5,  and  6  shows 
the  toe  areas  in  iR  to  be  higher6  than  the  thigh  area  and  in 
general  higher  than  the  foot  area  but  closely  approximating 
the  leg  area.  In  iL  the  area  is  massive  and  lies  nearer  the 
longitudinal  sulcus,  the  thigh  area,  with  the  exception  of  a  few 
points,  lying  lower  in  the  field.  For  2R  and  for  2L  similar 
statements  cannot  be  made,  for  in  general  the  toe  areas  of 
these  animals  lie  lower  down  than  the  areas  for  thigh  and  foot, 
although  they  approach  in  location  the  area  for  the  leg  move- 
ments. It  should,  however,  be  noted  that  in  these  hemispheres 
points  lying  much  lower  down  than  that  corresponding  to  the  toe 
area  were  found  for  the  thigh  and  leg  movements.  In  3R  the 
total  extensity  of  the  toe  area  is  much  less  than  that  for  any  of 
the  other  leg  elements  but  the  area  is  apparently  just  as  widely 
spread  over  the  cerebral  convexity.  In  3L  the  area  is  greater 
than  those  for  the  other  segments  and  to  a  slight  degree  it  is 
more  widely  spread.  In  4R  the  area  is  much  greater  and  extends 
higher  and  farther  backwards  than  the  other  areas.  In  5R 
the  area  is  the  least  extensive  of  the  four  leg  segments  and  it 
is,  unlike  that  for  the  foot  and  that  for  the  thigh,  compact.  5L 
is  also  an  area  without  divisions  and  is  more  compact  but  only 
slightly  smaller  than  the  other  three  areas  for  the  leg. 

It  is  to  be  noted,  therefore,  that  differences  exist  not  only 
with  respect  to  the  absolute  and  the  relative  sizes  of  this  area 
in  the  different  hemispheres  but  also  with  respect  to  the  diffusion 
or  compactness  of  the  area. 

At  the  same  time  mention  may  be  made  of  the  variations 
which  are  similar  to  those  which  have  already  been  described 
for  the  other  leg  segments.  We  find  the  absolute  amounts  of 

"  "Higher"  and  "lower"  here  refer  to  the  diagrams,  and  these  terms  cor- 
respond to  the  anatomical,  but  more  cumbersome,  "nearer  the  longitudinal 
sulcus"  and  "farther  from  the  longitudinal  sulcus"  respectively. 


n6 


SHEPHERD  IVORY  FRANZ 


the  areas  differing  in  the  different  animals,  and  at  the  same 
time  differing  in  the  two  hemispheres  of  the  same  animal.  More- 
over we  find  the  relative  amounts  of  the  areas  widely  different 
for  the  different  animals  and  for  the  two  hemispheres  of  the 
same  animal.  In  this  respect  there  is  an  agreement  with  the 
other  areas  which  have  previously  been  described.  In  iL,  3L, 
and  4R  the  toe  areas  exeed  those  for  the  thigh,  for  the  leg, 
and  for  the  foot;  in  2R,  2L,  58.,  and  5L  the  toe  areas  are 
exceeded  by  those  for  the  other  hind  limb  segments,  although 
the  excess  in  the  case  of  5L  is  slight  and  well  within  the  ob- 
servational error.  The  variations  in  totals  are  not  as  great  as 
those  which  have  been  noted  for  the  other  leg  segments,  nor  do 
the  percentages  in  relation  to  the  total  stimulable  areas  vary 
as  much.  The  hemisphere  to  hemisphere  variations,  with  the 
exception  of  those  of  the  brain  of  monkey  5,  are  considerable 
and  differ  in  some  cases  by  as  much  as  i  to  2.5.  The  results, 
diagrammatic  and  numerically,  are  given  in  Figure  6  and  in 
Table  VI. 

Shoulder. — The  totals  of  the  shoulder  areas  range  from  11.5 
sq.  mm.  (4R)  to  80.5  sq.  mm.  (2R),  with  percentages  in  rela- 
tion to  the  net  totals  (leg  and  arm  segments  together)  ranging 
from  15  (4R)  to  54  (3R).  The  differences  in  amount  of  the 

TABLE  VI.    Measurements  of  the  extents  of  toe  areas. 


Monkeys 

I 

2 

3 

4 

5 

Hemispheres 

R        L 

R        L 

R        L 

R 

R        L 

Areas  in  square 
mm. 

24.0      51.0 

20-5         52.5 

16.5      45-0 

33-5 

37-0      41-0 

Percentages 
R/L 

47 

39 

37 

— 

90 

Percentage  re- 
lations of  av- 
erages, Mon- 
key 2  =  IOO. 

103 

IOO 

84 

92 

168 

bo 
£ 

fi 


VARIATIONS  IN  DISTRIBUTION  OF  MOTOR  CENTERS      119 
TABLE  VII.    Measurements  of  the  extents  of  shoulder  areas. 


Monkeys 

I 

2 

3 

4 

5 

Hemispheres 

R        L 

R        L 

R        L 

R 

R        L 

Areas  in  square 
mm. 

37.0      25.0 

80.5      79-5 

67.5      32.0 

II-5 

26.0      48.0 

Percentages 
R/L 

148 

101 

211 

— 

54 

Percentage  re- 
lations of  av- 
erages, Mon- 
key 2  =  IOO. 

39 

IOO 

62 

H 

46 

areas  in  the  five  animals  are  greater  than  in  any  previous  area 
and  also  greater  than  any  of  the  areas  for  the  other  arm  seg- 
ments. The  amounts  and  the  illustrations  of  the  distributions 
of  these  areas  are  shown  in  Table  VII  and  in  Figure  7.  In 
relation  to  the  four  leg  segments  which  have  already  been  con- 
sidered there  are  four  hemispheres  in  which  the  shoulder  area 
exceeds  each  of  the  leg  segment  areas  (iR,  2R,  3R,  and  5L), 
there  are  two  hemispheres  in  which  the  shoulder  area  is  less 
than  each  of  the  leg  segment  areas  (4R  and  $R)  and  three 
hemispheres  in  which  two  of  the  leg  segment  areas  exceed  and 
the  other  two  are  less  than  the  shoulder  areas. 

The  variations  from  hemisphere  to  hemisphere  parallel  those 
which  have  already  been  discussed  for  other  segments,  the  great- 
est difference  being  found  in  monkeys  3  and  5,  where  the  dif- 
ferences are  approximately  100  per  cent. 

In  addition  to  the  differences  which  have  been  mentioned,  the 
wide-spread  distribution  of  the  shoulder  areas,  especially  in 
hemispheres  2L,  3R  and  5L,  is  noteworthy.  At  the  same  time 
the  discreteness  of  the  zones  is  a  prominent  feature  in  the 
illustrations.  There  is  also  to  be  noted  the  relation  of  the  areas 
to  the  central  fissure.  In  regard  to  this  it  will  be  observed 
that  the  areas  have  the  same  general  features  as  do  those  for  the 
thigh  movements  in  that  some  of  them  appear  to  be  outcropping 


120 


SHEPHERD  IVORY  FRANZ 


or  projections  from  the  concealed  fissural  areas,  whereas  others 
are  less  apparently  related  to  those  hidden  areas.  At  the  same 
time  in  some  hemispheres  there  is  an  apparent  sharp  definition 
of  the  areas  by  the  principal  blood  vessels  and  by  fissures  other 
than  that  of  Rolando.  While  from  the  facts  now  at  hand  it 
cannot  be  said  that  these  divisions  by  fissures  and  by  the  principal 
blood  vessels  have  any  special  physiological  significance  the 
finding  of  this  in  relation  to  several  areas  gives  an  indication 
that  these  landmarks  may  have  some  physiological  as  well  as 
anatomical  meaning.  An  examination  of  all  the  diagrams  is 
suggestive  of  this  conclusion,  but  the  matter  needs  a  more  ex- 
tensive and  a  more  careful  study  with  a  definite  question  in  view. 

Forearm. — The  apparent  outcropping  of  the  zones  for  the 
forearm  from  similar  areas  located  within  the  infoldings  of  the 
fissure  of  Rolando  is  more  noticeable  than  in  any  other  previously 
considered  segment.  In  every  hemisphere  there  is  a  considerable 
extent  of  forearm  area  bordering  upon  the  central  fissure  and 
moreover  the  areas  appear  more  solid  than  those  for  the  shoulder 
movements.  This  does  not  mean,  however,  that  the  phenomenon 
of  diffusion  is  lacking  for  in  fact  an  examination  of  the  diagrams 
of  Figure  8  reveals  a  marked  degree  of  separation  of  the  areas 
in  2R,  2L,  3R,  and  5R. 

Table  VIII  gives  the  measurements  of  these  forearm  areas 

TABLE  VIII.    Measurements  of  extents  of  forearm  areas. 


Monkeys 

I 

2 

3 

4 

5 

Hemispheres 

R        L 

R        L 

R        L 

R 

R        L 

Areas  in  square 
mm. 

55-0      56.0 

89-5      92-5 

80.5      63.5 

24.0 

54-0      52.5 

Percentages 
R/L 

98 

97 

127 

— 

103 

Percentage  re- 
lations of  av- 
erages, Mon- 
kev  2  =  TOO. 

61 

IOO 

79 

26 

59 

122 


SHEPHERD  IVORY  FRANZ 


in  the  nine  hemispheres.  Here  are  shown  almost  as  great  dif- 
ferences as  have  been  shown  to  exist  for  the  other  areas  already 
dealt  with,  for  in  one  case  (monkey  4)  we  find  the  area  only 
about  one-quarter  the  size  of  that  in  monkey  2.  In  two  animals 
(monkeys  I  and  5)  the  areas  are  not  only  nearly  equal  but  the 
two  hemispheres  are  also  approximately  of  the  same  size.  This 
is  the  closest  correspondence  which  is  to  be  found  in  the  whole 
series  but  I  hesitate  to  conclude  that  it  has  any  great  significance. 

In  three  animals  the  proportions  R/L  are  nearly  equal  (monk- 
eys i,  2  and  5)  which  again  is  a  condition  not  found  for  any 
other  area,  but  which  I  also  doubt  has  any  well  marked  signifi- 
cance. The  reason  for  this  is  to  be  sought  in  the  varying  rela- 
tions of  the  extents  of  these  areas  to  the  net  total  (leg  plus 
arm  segments)  areas.  When  this  comparison  is  made  it  is  to  be 
observed  that  no  close  correspondence  exists  except  for  the  two 
hemispheres  of  monkey  5.  By  this  comparison  the  two  forearm 
areas  of  monkey  i  differ  by  25  per  cent.,  those  of  monkey 
2  by  an  almost  equal  amount,  and  the  differences  in  the  relations 
of  the  hemispheres  of  monkey  3  are  also  approximately  equal. 

Hand. — The  data  regarding  the  extensions  of  the  areas  con- 
trolling movements  of  the  hand  are  given  in  Table  IX  and  in 
Figure  9.  Unlike  many  of  the  areas  which  have  previously 
been  considered  these  areas  are  not  massed  but  are  widespread 

TABLE  IX.    Measurements  of  extents  of  hand  areas. 


Monkeys 

i 
i 

2 

3 

4 

5 

Hemispheres 

R        L 

R        L 

R        L 

R 

R        L 

Areas  in  square 
mm. 

39-0       39.5 

42.0      58.5 

37-0      18.5 

II.O 

32.0      16.5 

Percentages 
R/L 

99 

72 

200 

— 

193 

Percentage  re- 
lations of  av- 
erages, Mon- 
key 2  =  TOO. 

78 

IOO 

55 

22 

48 

124  SHEPHERD  IVORY  FRANZ 

and  divided  relatively  more  than  any  of  the  leg  segment  areas 
and  the  arm  segment  areas  with  the  exception  of  those  for 
foot  movements.  The  average  extent  of  this  area  for  all  hemi- 
spheres (32.7  sq.  mm.)  is  less  than  that  of  the  other  segment 
areas  with  the  exception  of  the  foot  areas  (average  31.7  sq.  mm.) 
and  the  variations  are  considerable.  Although  the  average  ex- 
tent of  this  area  is  among  the  smallest  it  has  wide  variations, 
for  in  monkey  i  it  is  exceeded  in  each  hemisphere  by  but  two 
of  the  other  areas,  in  monkey  2  it  is  exceeded  in  each  hemisphere 
by  four  other  areas,  in  monkey  3  it  is  exceeded  on  the  right 
by  three  areas  but  on  the  left  by  six  areas,  in  the  one  hemisphere 
of  monkey%  4  it  is  exceeded  in  extent  by  seven  areas,  and  in 
monkey  5  on  the  right  by  seven  areas  and  on  the  left  by  all 
other  areas. 

The  variations  in  absolute  size  in  different  hemispheres  are 
great,  from  n  (4R)  to  58.5  sq.  mm.  (2L),  a  ratio  of  1:5.3. 
Its  relation  to  the  net  total  is  less  variable,  from  n  (sL)  to 
34  (iR),  although  the  ratio  is  slightly  greater  than  1:3.  In 
only  one  animal  (monkey  i)  are  the  areas  for  the  two  hemi- 
spheres nearly  equal  in  size,  although  in  relation  to  the  net 
totals  the  equality  is  greater  in  a  second  animal  (monkey  2). 

The  percentage  relations  of  R/L  show  only  one  instance  which 
has  previously  been  considered  (shoulder  area  of  monkey  3) 
in  which  the  difference  is  as  great  as  is  found  in  monkeys  3  and 
5  for  the  hand  areas.  These  figures  should,  however,  be  con- 
sidered to  be  no  more  than  suggestive  for  in  many  previous 
cases  (for  example,  thigh  areas  of  monkeys  i  and  2,  foot  areas 
of  monkey  i  and  3,  toe  areas  of  monkeys  i,  2  and  3,  etc.)  if 
the  reverse  percentages  L/R  had  been  used  as  a  basis  of  com- 
parison the  differences  would  have  been  much  greater  in  these 
other  hemispheres. 

The  apparent  punctiform  character  of  the  area  under  con- 
sideration is  obvious  in  a  number  of  the  hemispheres.  Although 
exceeded  in  size  in  most  instances  by  the  other  areas  the  number 
of  divisions  greatly  exceeds  those  for  the  leg  and  toes,  slightly 
exceeds  those  for  the  thigh  and  fingers,  and  is  exceeded  only 
slightly  by  the  foot  and  forearm  areas  and  to  a  greater  degree 


by  the  shoulder  areas.  For  such  a  relatively  small  area  this 
wide-spread  distribution  is  noteworthy. 

In  form  the  areas  under  consideration  are  widespread,  the 
scattering  being  such  as  to  make  the  different  hemispheres  ap- 
pear to  be  without  resemblance,  and  this  statement  is  true  for 
the  two  hemispheres  of  the  same  animal  as  well  as  for  the 
hemispheres  of  different  animals.  The  relation  of  the  areas 
to  the  central  fissure  is  not  constant,  not  more  than  one-half 
of  the  areas  in  the  nine  hemispheres  having  close  association 
with  this  fissure.  With  the  exception  of  a  slight  relation  to 
a  subsidiary  fissure  in  2L  the  areas  do  not  appear  to  bear  a 
close  relation  to  the  other  fissures  and  principal  blood  vessels, 
and  to  employ  again  the  figure  of  speech  which  has  previously 
been  used  it  may  be  said  that  most  of  the  cortex  which  re- 
sponded with  movements  of  the  hand  appears  to  be  outcroppings 
from  the  depths  or  upward  projections  from  possible  underlying 
areas. 

A  comparison  of  the  total  areas  in  Table  IX  with  those  in 
Tables  VII  and  VIII  shows  that  in  all  hemispheres  the  hand 
area  is  less  in  extent  than  that  for  the  forearm  and  in  monkeys 
2  and  3  less  than  that  for  the  shoulder,  but  in  monkey  I  it  is 
greater  than  that  of  the  shoulder  area  while  in  monkeys  4  and  5 
the  relations  are  irregular  or  undecided. 

If  the  data  which  have  been  given  were  to  be  interpreted  in 
a  manner  which  is  not  infrequent  we  might  conclude  from 
the  relative  measurements  that  in  general  the  hand  of  these 
monkeys  has  only  the  same  amount  of  cerebral  control  as  the 
foot,  but  that  in  specific  instances  the  hand  has  a  greater  amount 
of  cerebral  control  and  in  other  cases  the  foot  is  the  part  best 
represented  in  the  cortex.  Such  a  conclusion  appears  to  me 
obviously  premature,  for  even  though  it  may  prove  to  be  true, 
at  present  we  do  not  know  exactly  what  cortical  stimulability 
means  and  I  believe  we  should  not  conclude  from  a  comparison  of 
two  sets  of  measurements  that  we  are  dealing  with  information 
regarding  lesser  or  greater  cerebral  control.  The  movements  of 
the  hand  areas,  it  will  later  be  noted,  are  more  often  associated 
with  movements  of  the  other  arm  segments  than  are  those  of  the 


126 


SHEPHERD  IVORY  FRANZ 


foot  with  the  other  leg  segments,  and  the  matter  of  greater  or 
lesser  cerebral  control  appears  to  me  to  be  bound  up  with  the 
character  of  the  distribution  as  well  as  with  the  superficial  extents 
of  the  areas  from  which  such  movements  may  be  produced  by 
stimulation  methods.  It  seems  to  me,  therefore,  that  the  element 
of  complexity  is  an  important  factor,  and  that  the  question  of  the 
greater  or  less  control  should  be  considered  only  in  the  light  of  all 
the  data  for  all  the  segments. 
Fingers. — Figure  10  and  Table  X  contain  the  results  relative 

TABLE  X.    Measurements  of  extents  of  finger  areas. 


Monkeys 

I 

2 

3 

4. 

5 

Hemispheres 

R        L 

R        L 

R        L 

R 

V  R       L 

Areas  in  square 
mm. 

55-5      15-0 

40.0      57-5 

41.0      46.5 

IO.O 

35-5      40.5 

Percentages 
R/L 

370 

70 

89 

— 

88 

Percentage  re- 
lations of  av- 
erages, Mon- 
key 2  =  IOO. 

72 

IOO 

90 

21 

78 

to  the  areas  for  finger  movements.  Although  some  of  these 
areas  are  smaller  than  the  corresponding  areas  for  the  hand  they 
average  about  fifteen  per  cent  more  than  the  latter.  In  only  a 
few  cases  are  the  differences  great  enough  to  warrant  note, 
the  variations  in  hemispheres  iL  and  3L  and  5L  being  the 
greatest. 

The  comparative  range  of  the  areas  for  all  hemispheres  is 
approximately  i :  6,  which  is  nearly  the  same  proportion  obtained 
for  most  of  the  areas  which  have  previously  been  dealt  with. 
The  diagrams  of  Figure  10  show  the  closer  relation  of  the  areas 
to  the  central  fissure  than  is  to  be  found  with  some  of  the  other 
areas,  since  comparatively  large  portions  of  this  area  border 
upon  this  fissure.  This,  in  other  cases,  has  been  considered  to  be 
an  indication  of  the  possibility  that  large  or  small  cortical  areas 


" 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 '       '       ' L 


128 


SHEPHERD  IVORY  FRANZ 


for  such  movements  may  be  concealed  within  the  Rolandic  fis- 
sure. If  this  be  true  the  sizes  of  the  finger  areas  would  be  much 
greater  than  those  which  are  shown  in  the  diagram  and  in  the 
table. 

General. — For  purposes  of  comparison  I  think  we  are  justi- 
fied in  combining  the  totals  of  the  stimulable  areas  regardless  of 
overlapping,  especially  since  these  totals  in  relation  to  the  net 
totals  will  give  some  indications  of  the  extents  of  the  overlappings 
from  area  to  area,  and  consequently  they  also  give  indications  of 
the  complexities  of  the  movements  of  the  different  hemispheres. 
The  general  relations  are  shown  in  Table  XL  In  this  table 

TABLE  XI.  The  overlapping  of  areas  for  the  leg  and  arm  segments.  The 
totals  of  all  areas  were  obtained  by  adding  the  totals  for  each  hemisphere, 
and  the  amounts  of  overlapping  by  subtracting  from  these  figures  the  "net 
totals"  given  in  Table  II. 


I 

2 

3 

4 

5 

Hemispheres 

R         L 

R         L 

R         L 

R 

R         L 

Totals     of     all 

249-5  256.0 

411.0  589.0 

294.0  287.0 

155-0 

348.0    333-0 

areas 

i 

Net   totals 

H5-0  157.0 

152.0  215.0 

124.0   132.0 

78.0 

139.0  144.0 

Overlappings 

134-5    99-0 

259-0  374-0 

170-0  155.0 

77.0 

209.0  189.0 

Per  cent,  over- 
lappings  in  re- 
lation   to    net 

117         63 

170        174 

137        117 

99 

150          131 

totals 

are  shown  (a)  the  totals  of  the  areas  which  have  been  considered, 
that  is  the  sum  totals  of  the  thigh,  the  leg,  the  foot,  etc.,  areas 
for  all  nine  hemispheres,  (fc)  the  net  totals,  that  is  the  amount 
of  superficial  space  covered  by  the  areas,  (c)  the  differences 
between  these  sets  of  figures,  which  give  the  total  amounts  of 
overlapping  of  the  individual  areas,  and  (</)  the  percentage 
relations  of  the  amounts  of  overlappings  to  the  net  totals  of  the 
hemispheres.  It  is  obvious  that  if  the  series  of  stimuli  on  one 
hemisphere  produced  a  combined  movement  of  all  the  segments 
which  we  have  been  considering  there  would  be  a  total  of  over- 


VARIATIONS  IN  DISTRIBUTION  OF  MOTOR  CENTERS      129 

lapping  amounting  to  700  per  cent  of  the  net  total  for  that 
hemisphere,  because  each  of  the  segments  would  be  totally  repre- 
sented in  the  grand  total.  The  greater  percentage  of  overlapping 
is,  therefore,  an  indication  of  greater  complexity  of  movement. 
In  the  next  section  we  shall  deal  with  the  special  overlappings  of 
the  arm  segments  in  the  leg  area  and  of  the  leg  segments  in  the 
arm  areas  and  here  we  shall  confine  ourselves  to  the  consideration 
of  the  special  overlappings  of  the  arm  segments  among  themselves 
and  of  the  leg  segments  among  themselves.  This  includes  at  the 
same  time  the  borderline  overlappings,  since  these  border  areas 
cannot  be  considered  to  be  distinctively  either  arm  or  leg  areas. 

The  smallest  amounts  of  overlapping  was  found  in  4R  and  in 
iL,  the  greatest  in  2L  and  in  2R.  The  differences  in  this  respect 
range  from  77  to  374  sq.  mm.,  or  approximately  I  :  5.  In  rela- 
tion to  the  net  totals,  probably  a  fairer  means  of  comparison  of 
the  individual  hemispheres  with  one  another,  the  range  is  from 
63  to  174  per  cent,  or  approximately  i  :  3.  An  interesting  fact 
is  that  in  iR,  in  which  it  has  been  noted  (p.  96)  that  no  over- 
lapping of  leg  and  arm  areas  occurred,  the  total  amount  of  the 
overlapping  exceeded  that  of  4R,  in  which  leg-arm  overlapping 
was  found,  and  the  total  amount  of  the  overlapping  is  not  marked- 
ly less  in  iR  than  in  3L  in  which  the  amount  of  overlapping  of 
the  leg  and  arm  segment  areas  is  considerable.  At  the  same 
time  the  percentage  relations  give  equally  interesting  figures 
regarding  the  same  thing,  for  it  is  to  be  observed  that  the  per- 
centage of  overlapping  in  iR  is  greater  than  that  of  4R,  equal 
to  that  of  3L  and  is  not  greatly  exceeded  by  that  of  5L,  in  all 
of  which  hemispheres  the  amount  of  overlapping  of  the  leg-arm 
areas  is  not  especially  small.  This  indicates  that  in  hemisphere 
iR  there  has  been  a  more  general  complexity  of  movement  for 
the  two  segments  we  have  considered  than  for  the  other  hemis- 
pheres just  mentioned  since  a  certain  percentage  of  the  over- 
lappings  in  the  other  hemispheres  is  due  to  combinations  of  arm 
and  leg  movements.  The  low  percentage  in  iL  may  be  considered 
a  typical  example  of  what  may  be  expected  when  the  two  seg- 
ments have  not  overlapped. 

The  complications  or  the  combinations  of  movements  are  also 


130 


SHEPHERD  IVORY  FRANZ 


shown  by  the  number  of  cases  in  which  the  areas  for  one  segment 
overlap  those  of  the  other  segments.  These  data  for  the  leg 
segments  are  shown  in  Table  XII  and  those  for  the  arm  segments 

TABLE  XII.  Overlappings  of  leg  segment  areas.  Digits  represent  the 
numbers  of  hemispheres  in  which  overlapping  occurred.  The  total  possi- 
bilities are  nine  in  each  case. 


Segments 

Toes 

Foot 

Leg 

Thigh 

Toes 

— 

8 

8 

7 

Foot 

8 

9 

9 

Leg 

8 

9 

— 

9 

Thigh 

7 

9 

9 

— 

in  Table  XIII.    In  these  tables  there  are  shown  the  total  numbers 

TABLE  XIII.  Overlappings  of  arm  segment  areas.  Digits  represent  the 
numbers  of  hemispheres  in  which  overlapping  occurred.  The  total  possi- 
bilities are  nine  for  each  case. 


Segments 

Shoulder 

Forearm 

Hand 

Fingers 

Shoulder 

— 

9 

9 

8 

Forearm 

9 

— 

9 

9 

Hand 

9 

9 

— 

9 

Fingers 

8 

9 

9 

— 

of  cases  in  which  Overlappings  occurred,  not  the  individual  points 
in  the  areas,  and  consequently  not  the  totals  of  the  areas  or  the 
total  portions  of  the  cortex  representing  the  areal  distribution 
of  movements  of  the  segments.  If  one  area,  for  example  that  of 
the  shoulder,  should  have  an  overlapping  in  all  hemispheres  it 
is  clear  that  the  table  would  show  a  total  overlapping  in  nine 
hemispheres.  Similarly  for  the  other  segments.  When  the  num- 
ber is  less  than  nine,  it  means  that  in  one  or  more  hemispheres 
at  no  time  in  the  whole  series  of  experiments  upon  those  hemi- 
spheres did  combined  movements  of  the  two  segments  occur. 
The  tables  show  that  in  two  cases  (iR  and  iL)  there  was  no 
overlapping  of  the  thigh  and  the  toe  areas,  in  one  case  (iR)  there 
was  no  overlapping  of  the  foot  and  toe  areas,  and  in  one  case 
(iL)  there  was  no  overlapping  of  the  toe  and  leg  areas.  In 
view  of  the  fact  that  in  iR  no  overlapping  took  place  of  thigh 


VARIATIONS  IN  DISTRIBUTION  OF  MOTOR  CENTERS      131 

and  toe,  and  foot  and  toe,  the  small  percentages  of  overlapping  is 
understandable,  and  this  is  the  more  noticeable  also  because  of 
the  failure  to  find  from  cortical  stimulation  in  this  animal  com- 
bined movements  of  the  arm  and  leg. 

The  almost  universal  overlapping  in  the  arm  segments  areas 
indicates  the  general  complexity  of  the  movements  which  were 
obtained  by  the  stimulation  of  the  cortex  in  this  area,  and  at  the 
same  time  it  shows  that  the  complexity  is  found  in  all  hemispheres 
with  the  exception  of  shoulder-finger  relations  of  hemispheres 
iL.  It  does  not  show,  however,  that  there  is  the  same  degree  of 
complexity  for  all  other  hemispheres,  for  to  settle  the  latter  matter 
there  must  be  a  comparison  of  the  individual  points  which  it  is 
not  possible  to  make  at  this  time.  It  may  be  said,  however,  that 
there  are  considerable  variations  in  complexity  (hemisphere  and 
animal)  shown  by  the  examination  of  the  protocols  of  the  indi- 
vidual tests,  and  these  are  partly  indicated  by  the  differences  in 
the  totals  of  overlappings  which  are  shown  in  Table  XI.  It  may 
be  repeated  that  the  results  in  Tables  XII  and  XIII  do  not  mean 
that  there  has  been  a  total  overlapping  of  all  segments  in  all 
hemispheres  for  all  the  points  which  were  stimulated.  In  fact 
from  Table  XI  we  can  conclude  that  at  the  most  (2L)  there 
has  been  less  than  two-thirds  of  the  total  possible  amount  of 
overlapping  and  in  most  cases  the  amount  of  the  overlapping  is 
not  more  than  two-sevenths  of  the  possible  total,  and  usually 
much  less  than  this  amount.  If  there  were  no  overlap] pings  of 
arm  and  leg  segments  and  there  was  a  complete  overlapping  of 
the  areas  for  the  individual  parts  of  the  segments,  which  would 
mean  that  when  any  motor  point  was  stimulated  the  resulting 
reaction  would  be  a  movement  of  thigh  +  leg  +  foot  +  toes,  or 
a  movement  of  shoulder  -f~  forearm  -\-  hand  -f-  fingers,  we 
should  have  a  total  overlapping  of  300  per  cent.  A  considerable 
amount  of  overlapping  occurs  in  relation  to  the  leg  and  arm 
segment  areas  so  that  the  greatest  recorded  amounts  (2L,  374 
per  cent)  are  made  up  of  extra-segmental  overlappings  and  of 
inter-segmental  overlappings.  Table  II,  column  6  (p.  98) 
shows  the  amount  of  extra-segmental  overlappings,  and  the  fig- 
ures should  be  subtracted  from  those  in  Table  XI.  It  should  also 


132  SHEPHERD  IVORY  FRANZ 

be  remembered  that  the  figures  in  Table  II  do  not  represent  the 
totals  of  extra-segmental  overlappings  but  only  those  of  total-leg 
and  total-arm,  for  such  overlappings  may  be  of  two  or  more  parts 
of  each  of  the  two  segments  (arm  and  leg)  with  which  we  have 
been  dealing.  When,  however,  the  subtraction  which  is  sug- 
gested has  been  made  we  find  that  there  is  a  noticeable  reduc- 
tion in  some  of  the  figures  in  Table  XI.  At  the  same  time  it 
should  not  be  thought  that  in  dealing  with  the  relation  of  the 
cerebral  cortex  to  complexities  of  movement  such  subtractions 
should  be  made,  for  in  general  it  is  true  that  the  greater  the 
overlappings  the  greater  is  the  degree  of  movement  complexity. 

Thus,  stimulation  of  the  cortex  at  the  border  of  the  two 
large  areas  produced  complex  movements  of  the  two  totalized 
segments.  We  found  for  example  movements  of  all  the  arm 
segments  towards  the  leg  and  at  the  same  time  complementary 
movements  of  the  leg  towards  the  arm.  Such  movements  are 
well  represented  by  those  of  the  intact  animal  when  he  wishes 
to  scratch  his  leg,  but  makes  only  part  of  the  movement,  that 
pertaining  to  the  approach  of  the  hand  and  fingers  to  the  leg 
and  a  similar  approach  of  the  leg  towards  the  hand  so  that  the 
latter  has  a  better  chance  for  scratching.  Also,  such  a  movement 
as  the  transfer  of  food  from  the  hand  to  the  foot  is  of  this  com- 
plex type,  and  similarly  with  movements  which  simulate  or 
resemble  those  of  the  simultaneous  use  of  the  arm  and  leg 
segments  in  the  act  of  taking  hold  of  the  bars  of  the  cage.  Such 
movements  are  on  the  other  hand  not  always  simultaneus,  not 
infrequently  it  was  found  that  after  the  arm  had  made  a  definite 
movement  the  leg  would  make  a  movement  succeeding  that  of 
the  arm. 

Summary.' — The  data  show  that  in  different  animals  and  in 
different  hemispheres  a  variety  of  distribution  of  the  areas  con- 
cerned with  the  movements  of  the  individual  segments  of  the  leg 
and  arm.  In  addition  to  this  variation  in  distribution,  varia- 
tions in  the  total  amounts  of  the  different  areas  were  found, 
which  when  averaged  for  all  hemispheres  show  the  forearm  area 
of  greatest  size,  with  the  foot  area  of  the  smallest  average  size, 
and  between  these  extremes  and  in  serial  order  the  areas  for 


VARIATIONS  IN  DISTRIBUTION  OF  MOTOR  CENTERS      133 

the  shoulder,  the  leg,  the  thigh,  the  fingers,  the  toes,  and  the 
hand.  It  will  be  noticed  that  in  all  cases  the  average  of  a 
segment  of  the  arm  exceeded  in  areal  size  that  of  the 
corresponding  segment  of  the  hind  limb,  i.e.,  the  area  for  the 
shoulder  movements  was  on  the  average  greater  than  that  for 
thigh  movements,  that  for  'forearm  movements  greater  than 
that  for  leg  movements,  etc.  In  the  individual  hemispheres 
such  relations  do  not  exist,  in  some  cases  the  leg  areas 
being  correspondingly  larger  than  the  arm  areas.  At  the  same 
time  the  serial  order  for  the  segments  is  not  the  same  for  all 
animals,  in  one  case  the  thigh  area  being  the  largest,  in  another 
hemisphere  the  forearm  area  being  the  largest,  etc.  The  shapes 
of  the  corresponding  areas  in  the  nine  hemispheres  did  not  ap- 
proximately correspond  nor  was  there  a  correspondence  of  the 
spatial  relations  of  the  areas  to  such  well  marked  anatomical 
landmarks  as  the  central  fissure,  the  longitudinal  sulcus,  etc.  In  a 
few  cases  the  anatomical  dividing  lines  appeared  to  have  a  cer- 
tain physiological  significance,  but  in  other  cases,  and  especially 
in  relation  to  certain  areas,  this  was  not  indicated.  In  all  hemis- 
pheres excepting  those  of  one  animal  there  was  found  a  greater 
or  less  overlapping  of  the  areas  for  the  leg  and  arm  segment 
movements,  which  was  shown  by  the  production  of  combined 
reactions  of  these  segments.  In  all  hemispheres  without  excep- 
tion there  were  found  overlappings  of  the  areas  for  the  in- 
dividual segments  of  the  leg  and  arm,  which  was  shown  by 
the  complex  movements  of  these  parts.  These  overlappings 
varied  in  all  hemispheres,  by  which  is  meant  that  the  stimulation 
of  the  cortex  of  some  hemispheres  resulted  in  more  of  the 
complex  movements  than  did  that  of  others. 


IV.     ANOMALOUS  DISTRIBUTIONS  OF  THE  STIMU- 

LABLE  AREAS 

Arm  movements  within  the  leg  areas. — In  the  broadest  sense 
we  may  consider  that  the  leg  segment  area  is  that  area  within 
which  stimulation  produces  leg  movements,  and  the  arm  area  that 
area  within  which  stimulation  produces  arm  movements.  Taking 
this  definition  of  the  areas  we  should  need  to  consider  that  the  leg 
areas  or  the  arm  areas  are  not  massed  but  are  spread  over  the 
precentral  cortex  as  widely  as  the  zone  in  which  a  stimulation 
at  any  point  produces  such  movements.  For  a  better  limitation  of 
the  areas  we  must  not  deal  with  the  widely  spreading  areas  in 
this  manner,  but  limit  the  designation  of  the  individual  areas  to 
those  solid-like  combinations  of  zones  in  which  the  special  char- 
acter of  movements  are  uniformly  or  nearly  uniformly  obtained. 
By  thus  dealing  with  our  results  we  can  say  that  in  general  there 
is  a  leg  area  separate  and  distinct  from,  but  contiguous  to,  the 
arm  area,  and  that  there  is  a  similar  arm  area,  but  that  between 
the  two  areas  there  is  an  intermediate  zone  which  is  allied  to 
both,  or  which  is  both,  and  which  can  be  considered  to  be  a  com- 
bined arm  and  leg  area. 

By  the  limitation  or  the  definition  of  the  areas  in  this  manner 
we  find  that  there  remain  certain  areas  or  points  within  each  of 
the  principal  areas  which  are  associated  with  the  production  of 
movement  of  another  segment  separate  from  the  segment  with 
which  the  area  has  definite  connections.  Thus  we  find  that  arm 
movements  are  sometimes  produced  by  the  stimulation  of  areas 
which  are  enclosed  on  all  sides  by  zones  which  are  distinctively 
leg  movement  areas  since  only  leg  movements  are  produced  by  the 
stimulation  of  the  cortex  in  these  regions  and  at  the  same  time 
there  are  arm  areas  within  which  stimulation  sometimes  pro- 
duces leg  movements.  These  areas  are  different  from  the  border- 
ing areas  in  that  the  results  are  unlike  those  for  the  surrounding 
zones,  and  are  therefore  to  be  considered  anomalous,  while  the 
bordering  areas  are  "normally"  the  combinations  which  are 
expected  in  view  of  the  continuity  of  the  whole  stimulable  zone. 


VARIATIONS  IN  DISTRIBUTION  OF  MOTOR  CENTERS      135 

An  examination  of  Figure  2  shows  that  there  are  no  bordering 
combination  areas  in  the  hemispheres  of  monkey  i  but  that  such 
areas  are  found  in  all  the  other  hemispheres.  It  will  also  be 
observed  that  there  are  no  anomalously  distributed  areas  in  the 
hemispheres  of  monkey  I  and  none  in  2R,  but  that  the  other 
six  hemispheres  show  anomalous  areas.  In  2L  we  find  an  area 
of  this  character  with  a  considerable  extensity  having  its  upper 
border  contiguous  to  the  longitudinal  sulcus  and  extending  down- 
wards towards  the  fissure  of  Sylvius  along  the  principal  blood 
vessels  which  is  illustrated  in  the  diagram.  We  also  find  in  this 
hemisphere  a  small  area  which  is  related  to  arm  movement  con- 
trol, for  movements  of  the  shoulder  were  produced  by  the  stimu- 
lation of  this  area  although  the  stimulation  of  the  same  points 
gave  leg  movements  and  the  stimulation  of  the  surrounding  areas 
also  gave  purely  leg  movements.  Similar  zones  were  found  in 
the  hemispheres  of  monkey  3 ;  on  the  right  at  the  extreme  frontal 
border  of  the  area  points  were  found  the  stimulations  of  which 
were  followed  by  movements  of  the  forearm  as  well  as  of  the 
leg  segments,  and  on  the  left  side  a  similar  zone  in  the  center 
of  the  leg  area  the  stimulations  of  which  produced  simultaneous 
movements  of  the  segments  of  the  leg  and  of  the  shoulder.  In 
monkey  5  there  were  also  found  on  the  right  side  a  combined 
area  bordering  upon  the  longitudinal  sulcus  which  stimulations 
showed  was  associated  with  the  production  of  forearm  move- 
ments as  well  as  with  leg  movements,  and  a  second  area  some- 
what lower  in  the  field  which  gave  similar  combinations  of  fore- 
arm and  leg  movements,  in  the  left  hemisphere  of  this  animal 
the  combined  area  bordering  upon  the  longitudinal  sulcus  gave 
movements  of  the  shoulder  in  addition  to  the  leg  segment  move- 
ments which  were  noted  in  the  protocol  of  the  experiment  as 
follows:  "extension  of  toes,  followed  by  extension  of  the  leg 
and  thigh,  and  a  movement  of  the  tail  to  the  right,  with  a  mass 
movements  of  the  arm  such  as  is  made  when  lifting  the  shoulder." 

The  general  results  regarding  the  amounts  of  overlappings  of 
the  arm  and  leg  segment  areas  are  shown  in  Table  XIV.  This 
table  shows  only  the  general  view  of  the  relations  as  indicated  by 
the  overlappings  of  the  different  areas,  without  considering  the 


136 


SHEPHERD  IVORY  FRANZ 


TABLE  XIV.  Overlapping  of  leg  and  arm  segment  areas.  Digits  represent 
the  numbers  of  hemispheres  in  which  overlapping  occurred.  The  total  possi- 
bilities are  nine  in  each  case. 


Segments 

Toes 

Foot 

Leg 

Thigh 

Shoulder 

3 

4 

4 

5 

Forearm 

4 

4 

6 

5 

Hand 

I 

2 

3 

3 

Fingers 

o 

0 

2 

2 

spatial  character  of  the  overlappings.  Thus,  for  example,  the 
total  number  of  possible  overlappings  is  nine  in  each  case,  and 
we  find  that  in  no  case  do  we  get  an  overlapping  of  the  toe  area 
onto  the  finger  area,  that  in  general  the  segments  close  to  the 
trunk  show  the  greatest  number  of  overlappings,  both  with  re- 
spect to  one  another  and  also  with  respect  to  the  more  peripheral 
segments.  This  would  indicate  a  possible  closer  relation  of  the 
thigh  movements  and  the  movements  of  the  hand  and  fingers, 
and  a  possible  closer  relation  of  movements  of  the  shoulder  with 
movements  of  the  foot  and  toes.  Until  the  movements  are  ana- 
lyzed to  a  greater  degree  than  is  done  at  this  time  this  can  be 
taken  only  as  a  suggestion. 

Leg  movements  within  the  arm  areas. — The  number  of  anomal- 
ous movements  of  this  character  is  less  than  that  of  the  arm 
movements  in  the  leg  areas.  In  2L,  on  the  outer  border  of  the 
arm  area,  there  is  a  small  area  the  stimulation  of  which  produced 
movements  of  the  thigh  and  of  the  leg,  and  at  the  frontal  edge 
of  this  area  the  stimulation  was  followed  by  knee  flexions  inde- 
pendent of  any  movement  of  the  parts  of  the  arm  segment.  At 
the  lower  portion  of  the  arm  area,  far  removed  from  the  main 
mass  of  the  leg  area,  two  small  areas  were  discovered  which  gave 
movements  of  the  thigh  and  leg  as  well  as  movements  of  the  arm 
segments,  in  one  case  the  leg  movements  being  combined  with 
movements  of  the  hand  and  fingers  and  in  another  case  with  move- 
ments of  the  forearm.  In  4R  thigh  and  leg  movements  were 
found  to  accompany  stimulation  of  the  area  in  the  central  portion 
of  the  large  arm  area  where  the  latter  bordered  upon  the  fissure  of 
Rolando. 


VARIATIONS  IN  DISTRIBUTION  OF  MOTOR  CENTERS      137 

Only  two  hemispheres,  therefore,  showed  the  (presence  of 
leg  movements  within  the  arm  areas  while  five  hemispheres  of  the 
total  of  nine  showed  arm  movements  within  the  leg  areas.  Al- 
though it  is  not  possible  at  this  time  to  determine  the  import  of 
these  variations  it  appears  probable  that  they  indicate  a  greater 
degree  of  ease  of  liberation  of  the  arm  movement  impulse  or  a 
greater  degree  of  complexity  of  arm  connections.  Allied  to  the 
results  which  have  been  considered  in  the  preceding  paragraph 
similar  results  were  obtained  which  indicate  the  complexity  of 
these  movements  and  of  the  movement  control.  In  3R  head 
movements  in  combination  with  movements  of  the  shoulder  were 
obtained  by  the  stimulation  of  the  cortex  at  the  uppermost  part  of 
the  arm  area  although  the  characteristic  area  for  head  movements 
lies  lower  in  the  field,  approximately  below  that  for  the  arm  seg- 
ments. In  the  same  hemisphere  head  movements  were  found  to 
follow  the  stimulation  of  the  area  close  to  the  bifurcation  of  the 
subsidiary  fissure  which  is  shown  in  the  diagram.  This  is  far 
removed  from  the  principal  head  area.  At  the  same  time  men- 
tion may  be  made  of  the  fact  that  in  4R  the  stimulation  of  the 
area  which  is  shown  as  a  blank  space  bordering  upon  the  fissure 
of  Rolando,  and  which  is  surrounded  by  arm  areas,  also  gave 
movements  of  the  tail  whereas  in  the  other  animals  in  which 
movements  of  this  organ  occurred  they  followed  stimulations  of 
the  areas  bordering  upon  or  near  the  longitudinal  sulcus. 

Relatively  non-stimulable  zones. — In  the  diagrams  of  Figure  2 
there  are  to  be  found  blank  spaces  within  the  cross-lined  areas,  or 
separating  the  cross-lined  areas.  This  indicates  that  these  areas 
are  unlike  the  surrounding  areas  in  that  they  are  non-stimulable 
or  relatively  non-stimulable.  This  was  mentioned  in  a  previous 
section  (p.  84),  where  it  was  also  said  that  when  an  apparent 
non-stimulable  area  was  found  the  strength  of  the  stimulating 
current  was  increased  to  see  if  the  area  was  really  non-stimulable. 
At  times  it  was  found  that  we  were  dealing  with  an  area 
with  heightened  threshold  because  the  increased  stimulus 
produced  characteristic  responses  similar  to  those  of  the  sur- 
rounding regions,  but  at  other  times  the  increased  stimulus 
did  not  produce  any  response.  Whenever  the  latter  was 


i38  SHEPHERD  IVORY  FRANZ 

found  it  was  concluded  that  we  were  dealing  with  a  silent 
or  a  relatively  silent  area  on  account  of  the  fact  that  any 
further  increase  of  current  strength  can  be  objected  to  on  the 
ground  that  the  spreading  of  the  current  is  more  likely  to  take 
place  and  to  stimulate  not  only  at  the  spot  at  which  the  electrodes 
are  placed  but  also  adjacent  collections  of  cells.  Some  objection 
may  be  raised  against  the  universal  application  of  this  method 
of  determining  the  silent  character  of  the  areas,  and  I  do  not 
press  the  point  at  the  present  time  although  the  conclusion  appears 
to  me  to  harmonize  with  a  number  of  other  facts  which  have 
previously  been  reported  by  others.  Whatever  explanation  we 
may  select  for  the  findings,  whether  we  consider  them  to  be  indica- 
tive of  a  non-stimulable  character  of  the  special  regions  or  of  a 
relative  lowering  of  irritability,  it  is  of  special  interest  to  note 
that  similar  phenomena  were  not  observed  in  the  results  of  the 
series  of  stimuli  to  all  the  hemispheres  which  were  tested.  At  the 
same  time  in  a  comparison  of  the  hemispheres  for  which  this 
phenomenon  was  noted  there  is  found  great  variation.  Thus  we 
note  the  leg  area  in  iR  to  be  divided  into  smaller  areas,  which 
division  is  probably  of  the  character  described  above  although 
not  obviously  so.  The  amount  of  space  covered  by  this  dividing 
area  is  great  in  the  case  of  2R,  and  the  relatively  non-stimulable 
zone  in  this  hemisphere  almost  divides  the  arm  area  into  two 
separate  areas.  Fewer  of  these  non-stimulable  areas  were  found 
in  the  leg  areas  than  in  the  arm  areas,  which  may  be  an  indication 
of  a  greater  fixity  or  of  a  higher  degree  of  exactness  in  the 
development  for  the  former.  In  this  connection  the  diagrams 
of  the  other  figures  are  of  interest  since  they  show  similar  phe- 
nomena associated  with  the  areas  for  the  individual  segments. 
It  will  be  observed  that  in  most  of  the  diagrams  of  the  arm  areas 
(shoulder,  forearm,  etc.)  these  divisions  occur  and  that  they  are 
less  frequent  in  the  leg  areas.  It  may  be  expected  that  the  divi- 
sions would  be  more  evident  in  the  arm  segment  areas  on  account 
of  the  multiplication  of  the  blank  areas  in  each  of  the  diagrams, 
but  a  careful  comparison  of  all  the  diagrams  referring  to  the 
arm  areas  shows  that  the  divisions  are  more  numerous  than  those 
of  the  leg  areas  even  when  the  non-stimulable  zones  that  are 
common  to  all  are  omitted. 


VARIATIONS  IN  DISTRIBUTION  OF  MOTOR  CENTERS      139 

Summary:  In  the  hemispheres  which  were  investigated  there 
was  found  a  number  of  points,  or  areas,  the  stimulation  of  which 
gave  movements  unlike  those  of  the  surrounding  regions.  This 
was  especially  marked  in  some  of  the  hemispheres  while  others 
showed  none  of  this  crossing  or  combination  of  control.  The 
number  of  cases  in  which  arm  movements  were  found  to  be  asso- 
ciated with  the  stimulation  of  leg  areas,  or  arm  movements  asso- 
ciated with  leg  movements  when  the  surrounding  areas  gave  only 
leg  movements,  is  greater  than  that  of  arm  movements  from  the 
stimulation  of  leg  areas.  A  number  of  non-stimulable  or  rela- 
tively non-stimulable  zones  were  found  surrounded  by  normally 
stimulable  areas. 


GENERAL  DISCUSSION  (THEORETICAL) 

The  results  of  these  experiments  lead  to  certain  conclusions 
which  have  widespread  application  regarding  the  functions,  or 
functional  connections,  of  all  portions  of  the  cerebrum.  It  is 
apparent  that  in  this  area,  which  has  very  generally  been  con- 
sidered to  have  neural  connections  directly  (or  indirectly  through 
intervening  neurones)  with  the  efferent  cellular  elements  in  the 
spinal  cord,  there  is  not  the  definiteness  of  localization,  and  there- 
fore of  connection,  which  has  been  supposed.  The  fact  that  there 
is  a  variation  in  the  extent  of  the  leg  or  the  arm  area  in  different 
animals  indicates  that  motor  cells  located  in  similar  locations  may 
send  impulses  in  different  directions.  The  fact  that  the  stimula- 
tion of  certain  spatially  located  points  in  an  area  which  usually 
gives  rise  to  movements  of  the  thigh  may,  for  example,  result  in 
combined  movements  of  the  arm  as  well  as  of  the  thigh  is  also  an 
indication  of  a  complex  mechanism.  This  becomes  more  evident 
when  we  realize  that  such  combined  movements  are  obtainable 
upon  stimulation  of  the  cortex  of  one  animal  and  not  when  the 
cortex  of  another  is  stimulated  (or  if  obtained  in  the  second 
animal  the  combined  movement  differs  in  quality) .  Furthermore, 
although  the  data  in  regard  to  the  differences  of  control  from  the 
two  hemispheres  of  the  same  animal  are  not  as  complete  as  they 
might  be,  the  facts  from  the  four  monkeys  of  which  both  hemis- 
pheres were  investigated  indicate  that  there  is  a  considerable 
difference  in  the  connections  which  are  established  from  each 
hemisphere.  All  the  varying  results  are,  however,  of  interest  in 
that  they  lead  to  rather  definite  theoretical  conclusions  and  in 
that  they  lead  to  a  better  understanding  of  the  variability  in  con- 
trol which  are  evidenced  by  the  normal  behavior  of  different 
animals  and  by  that  of  different  individuals  and  of  different 
races  of  man. 

In  connection  with  the  results  of  the  present  series  of  tests,  the 
results  of  the  recent  experiments  of  Brown  and  Sherrington  (3) 


VARIATIONS  IN  DISTRIBUTION  OF  MOTOR  CENTERS      141 

on  the  reversibility  of  action  of  allied  centers7  in  the  cortex  are  of 
very  great  importance.  These  investigators  found  that  in  the 
monkey's  cerebral  cortex  there  were  several  centers,  or  groups  of 
cells,  the  stimulation  of  which  normally  brought  about  flexion,  and 
other  adjacent  groups  or  centers  which,  stimulation  showed,  were 
normally  concerned  in  the  production  of  extension  movements. 
The  stimulation  of  one  of  the  flexion  centers,  it  was  furthermore 
found,  would  bring  about  a  slight  or  medium  degree  of  flexion, 
while  that  of  another  would  produce  an  extreme  degree  of  flexion. 
Similar  results  were  obtained  upon  stimulation  of  the  extension 
centers,  or  groups  of  cells.  When,  however,  a  flexion  center 
was  stimulated  repeatedly  it  was  discovered  that  the  flexion  re- 
action did  not  remain  of  the  same  intensity  as  that  which  was 
originally  found.  There  were  changes  in  the  degree  of  the  mus- 
cular contractions  in  a  series  of  stimulations  of  the  same  cerebral 
spot  and  in  certain  cases  flexion  eventually  was  replaced  by  the 
opposed  movement  of  extension.  From  the  results  of  this  exper- 
iment we  see  that  the  repeated  stimulation  of  one  area  may  result 
in  a  reversal  of  the  function,  such  a  reversal,  however,  being 
probably  only  an  exaggeration  due  to  the  normal  connections  and 
perhaps  only  a  magnification  in  certain  respects  of  the  normal 
functions  of  such  a  center.  After  pointing  out  this  fact  the  au- 
thors conclude  (page  277)  that  "the  frequence  of  reversal  as  a 
phenomenon  attaching  to  the  reactions  of  points  in  the  motor 
cortex  suggests  that  one  of  the  functions  of  the  cortex  may  be 
the  performance  of  reversals,  and  that  the  greater  predominance 
of  reversal  under  cortical  than  in  purely  spinal  or  decerebrate 
reflexes  is  because  reversal  is  one  of  the  specific  offices  of  the 
cortex  cerebri." 

In  the  same  series  of  experiments  Brown  and  Sherrington  also 
noted  another  result  which  is  of  great  interest,  namely  the  varia- 
tion in  the  degree  of  activity  accompanying  the  stimulation  of  the 
different  centers  at  different  times.  When  on  the  cortex  of  an  ani- 

T  The  term  center  in  the  discussion  implies  no  metaphysical  assumption. 
It  is  a  convenient  and  short  designation  for  "a  collection  of  cells  the  stimu- 
lation of  which  may  result  in  certain  reactions  and  the  extirpation  of  which 
may  bring  about  certain  deficiencies  of  activities  or  behavior." 


142  SHEPHERD  IVORY  FRANZ 

mal  the  center  for  extreme  flexion  was  definitely  located  (i.e., 
spatially  in  relation  to  the  other  flexion  centers)  and  the  animal 
was  permitted  to  recover  for  some  hours  before  a  second  experi- 
mental determination  of  this  center  was  made,  it  was  discovered 
that  the  second  determination  of  the  point  for  extreme  flexion 
did  not  always  correspond  with  the  point  originally  determined. 
In  other  words,  repeated  stimulation  of  the  same  center,  or  group 
of  cells,  did  not  always  result  in  the  production  of  the  same 
quantity  or  grade  of  movement.  Thus,  the  center  which  on  the 
first  stimulation  gave  the  greatest  amount  of  reaction  might  be 
found  to  give  a  less  amount  of  reaction  at  another  time  and  the 
area  which  gave  the  small  amount  of  flexion  or  extension  at  the 
time  of  the  original  or  first  experiment  was  sometimes  found  to 
give  a  greater  amount  of  flexion  or  of  extension  in  a  second  test. 

A  phenomenon  or  reversal  of  another  character  was  also  found. 
When  the  stimulation  of  a  center  resulted  in  a  continued  or  epi- 
leptiform  contraction  a  second  stimulation  of  the  same  center 
might  cause  an  inhibition  of  this  movement.  In  the  same  series 
of  experiments  it  was  found  that  "in  one  case  the  same  point 
which  yielded  primary  extension  with  much  regularity,  on  re- 
examination  twenty-eight  hours  later  in  the  same  animal,  yielded 
at  first  primary  flexion  instead  of  the  primary  extension" 
(page  252). 

Closely  allied  to  the  results  of  the  present  work  is  the  fact, 
which  Brown  and  Sherrington  note,  that  "in  some  experiments, 
the  area  whence  extension  points  could  be  chosen  has  been 
distinctly  larger  than  in  others"  (page  252).  These  authors  con- 
clude that  "this  variability  signifies  less  a  difference  in  the  per- 
manent arrangement  than  a  difference  in  the  condition  of  the 
nervous  system  from  time  to  time,"  but  this  conclusion  does  not 
appear  to  be  in  line  with  the  facts  which  have  been  recorded  here, 
nor  does  it  appear  to  me  adequate  to  explain  all  of  the  facts  which 
these  authors  have  recorded. 

Most  of  the  recent  work  on  the  recovery  of  voluntary  control 
following  various  forms  of  nerve  anastomosis  also  shows  that 
there  is  not  the  degree  of  definiteness  of  control  from  a  particular 
portion  of  the  cerebral  cortex  which  has  been  assumed.  Were  a 


VARIATIONS  IN  DISTRIBUTION  OF  MOTOR  CENTERS      143 

particular  cell  endowed  with  the  particular  function  of  sending 
impulses8  to  bring  about  only  one  special  movement  of  the  arm, 
it  could  never  be  used  to  bring  above  a  movement  of  the  face. 
Were  the  activities  of  such  a  cell  associated  with  or,  as  some 
clinicians  hold,  due  to  "memory  images"  of  shoulder  movements, 
for  example,  there  would  probably  never  be  the  possibility  of  mov- 
ing the  face  except  by  thinking  of  it  as  the  shoulder.  In  man,  as 
far  as  our  knowledge  goes,  the  acquired  ability  to  move  the  face 
when  there  has  been  an  anastomosis  between  the  peripheral  por- 
tion of  the  facial  nerve  and  the  central  end  of  the  accessory  nerve 
is  not  associated  with  any  "memory  images"  or  thinking  about  the 
shoulder.  This  conclusion  is  also  forced  upon  us  because  of  the 
recovery  of  the  facial  mimetic  movements,  which  are  reflex  in 
character. 

The  experimental  work  on  animals  gives  us  many  facts  of  value 
in  this  connection.  Kennedy  (8),  it  will  be  remembered,  crossed 
the  nerves  for  the  flexor  and  the  extensor  movements  of  the  dog's 
leg  and  noted  that  after  a  time  the  animal  was  able  to  move  the 
leg  quite  properly.  He  also  found  that  when  the  motor  areas  of 
the  cerebrum  were  stimulated,  the  stimulation  of  the  portion  of  the 
cortex  which  is  considered  to  be  a  flexion  "center"  was  accom- 
panied by  an  extension,  and  vice  versa,  indicating  that  new  brain 
conections  had  been  formed  because  of  the  peripheral  anastomosis. 
The  observation  that  the  animal  recovered  to  a  very  great  extent 
the  normal  power  of  the  use  of  the  leg  in  locomotion  demonstrates 
that  there  has  been  a  rearrangement  in  anatomical  distribution  of 
the  individual  neurones.  The  results  of  the  cerebral  stimulation 
experiments  on  the  brain  of  this  dog  are,  however,  not  conclusive 
evidences  of  the  rearrangement  of  function  in  the  neighboring 
cerebral  areas  because  the  flexor  and  the  extensor  centers  are 
anatomically  very  intimately  related.  The  recent  work  of  Brown 
and  Sherrington,  which  has  already  been  described,  has  well 
demonstrated  that  there  may  be  a  reversal  of  function  of  these 
areas,  and  it  is  theoretically,  and  practically,  possible  that  the  re- 

8  The  use  of  the  term  impulse  in  this  connection  is  convenient,  although  it 
is  recognized  that  some  physiologists  object  seriously  to  its  use,  because  it 
appears  to  imply  something  more  than  physico-chemical  changes. 


144  SHEPHERD  IVORY  FRANZ 

suits  of  the  cerebral  stimulation  experiments  of  Kennedy  were 
due  to  the  normal  physiological  relationships  of  the  flexion  and 
the  extension  centers. 

When,  however,  we  deal  with  the  altered  innervations  of  parts 
which  are  not  as  closely  associated  physiologically  the  same  prob- 
abilities do  not  exist.  The  further  experiments  of  Kennedy  (7) 
are,  therefore,  less  open  to  question,  for  he  found  it  was  possible 
to  obtain  the  return  of  function  in  the  appropriate  parts  when  he 
connected  the  central  portion  of  the  cut  spinal  accessory  or  the 
central  portion  of  the  cut  hypoglossal  with  the  distal  portion  of 
the  cut  facial  nerve.  After  these  operations  it  was  found  that  the 
first  movements  in  the  area  innervated  normally  by  the  facial 
nerve  came  in  58  and  32  days  respectively,  and  in  about  100  days 
there  was  found  to  be  a  recovery  of  the  voluntary  control  of  the 
closure  of  the  eye  and  of  other  parts. 

Other  motor  areas  of  the  cerebral  cortex  which  are  spatially  less 
closely  allied  are  also  found  to  have  the  ability  to  assume  func- 
tions not  originally  pertaining  to  them.  Thus  Osborne  (9)  and 
Kilvington,  in  their  very  suggestive  research,  found  that  if  one 
brachial  plexus  was  served  and  some  strands  were  carried  over 
from  the  opposite  plexus  and  united  with  the  distal  parts  of  the 
one  which  had  been  severed  regeneration  of  the  nerve  took  place. 
This  nerve  regeneration  was  accompanied  by  a  complete,  or  almost 
complete,  return  of  function  in  both  forelimbs.  It  was  further- 
more determined  that  if  the  cerebral  cortex  in  the  forelimb  area 
was  stimulated  on  the  side  contralateral  to  the  completely  cut 
nerve  (which  normally  innervates  the  limb  supplied  by  the  nerves 
of  the  brachial  plexus  which  had  been  cut),  no  reaction  resulted, 
but  if  the  cortex  of  the  homolateral  hemisphere  was  stimulated, 
movements  of  both  forelimbs  were  produced.  This  is  a  clear  and 
convincing  demonstration  of  the  fact  that  the  function  of  a  par- 
ticular area  depends  more  upon  the  connections  that  are  made 
than  upon  any  hypothetical  inherent  or  innate  function,  and  fur- 
ther, that  the  functions  of  a  particular  area  may  materially  change 
in  accordance  with  the  paths  which  are  formed.  The  recovery 
of  normal  function  also  indicates  that  the  impulses  received  from 
the  receptive  areas  of  the  cerebrum,  which  are  considered  to  be 


VARIATIONS  IN  DISTRIBUTION  OF  MOTOR  CENTERS      145 

necessary  for  the  proper  performance  of  voluntary  movements, 
are  not  singularly  direct.  Such  impulses,  following  the  assump- 
tion of  their  necessity  and  importance,  must  in  an  intact  animal 
go  in  certain  directions  and  in  the  operated  animal  in  other  direc- 
tions by  other  paths. 

The  experimental  work  of  Boeke  (1)  is  also  of  suggestive  im- 
portance, showing  as  it  does  that  there  is  a  possibility  of  regenera- 
tion even  in  those  cases  in  which  the  cross-sutured  nerves  differ 
by  as  much  as  they  do  in  the  case  of  the  sensory  and  the  motor 
nerves.  In  some  cases  Boeke  found  that  if  the  central  end  of 
the  cut  hypoglossal  nerve  was  joined  to  the  distal  portion  of  the 
sectioned  lingual  rierve  regeneration  of  the  fibers  took  place.  He 
has  also  been  able  to  demonstrate  that  under  these  conditions  some 
of  the  efferent  fibers  of  the  hypoglossal  actually  progressed  to  the 
surface  of  th'e  tongue  and  made  connections  with  taste  buds.9 
It  is  not  definitely  proven  that  these  connections  resulted  in  a 
return  of  the  ability  to  taste  for  those  areas  of  the  tongue  which 
had  been  deprived  of  his  function  by  the  section  of  the  lingual 
nerve.  More  experiments  and  more  crucial  tests  respecting  this 
matter  are  necessary  before  we  may  say  there  has  been  a  complete 
demonstration  of  the  functional  regeneration.  The  fact  remains, 
however,  that  the  possibility  of  anastomosis  between  a  sensory  and 
a  motor  nerve  has  been  demonstrated.  What  variations  in  sen- 
sory or  motor  activity  have  been  the  result  of  these  tests  will 
doubtless  later  be  determined.  That  the  normally  efferent  fibers 
may  regenerate  and  pass  to  the  sensory  end  organs  as  well  as  to 
muscle  cells  is  a  fact  weighted  with  suggestions  regarding  many 
practical,  but  especially  theoretical,  problems. 

Variations  in  the  motor  responses  to  cerebral  stimuli  have  also 
been  recorded  by  the  Vogts  (H),  not  only  in  different  animals 
of  the  same  species  but  also  in  different  species  of  animals.  It 
seems  to  me  probable  that  many  of  the  discussions  of  the 
physiologists  and  of  the  clinicians  of  the  past  in  which  there 

*At  the  present  stage  work  of  this  nature  has  more  definite  interest  in 
connection  with  the  peripheral  distribution  of  the  nerve  fibers.  It  can  readily 
be  understood,  however,  that  the  possible  central  (i.e.,  cerebral)  relations  are 
most  important.  Confirmations  of  Boeke's  work  are  urgently  needed,  espe- 
cially in  relation  to  the  variations  in  behavior  of  the  operated  animals. 


I46  SHEPHERD  IVORY  FRANZ 

were  charges  and  counter-charges  of  ignorance,  or  of  misstate- 
ment,  or  of  technical  defects  were  due  to  the  fact  that  these 
variations  were  not  known  or  were  not  recognized.  I  believe  that 
if  this  fact  had  been  known  and  understood  many  acrimonious 
discussions  would  have  been  prevented. 

The  Vogts  hold  to  the  view  that  the  variations  are  due  "in  part 
to  special  development  of  other  pallium  fields,  and  in  part  to  varia- 
itons  in  the  functional  capacity  of  performance."  They  further- 
more assert  that  nothing  prevents  the  "connecting  causally  all 
variations  in  the  number  of  foci  and  in  the  extension  and  to  a 
certain  degree  also  variations  in  the  excitability  of  a  certain  field 
of  stimulation  with  differences  in  the  specialization  of  its  motor 
functions." 

On  the  other  hand  Sherrington  (10)  has  written:  "Every  in- 
crease in  the  number  of  links  composing  the  nerve  cell  chain  seems 
to  increase  greatly  the  uncertainty  of  its  reaction  in  artificial  ex- 
citation. ...  A  cortex  cerebri  might  well  therefore  have  been 
expected  to  yield  under  artificial  excitation  only  extraordinarily 
inconstant  results.  To  Hitzig  and  Fritsch,  and  to  Ferrier,  we 
owe  the  pregnant  demonstration  that  as  regards  the  motor  region 
this  expectation  is  not  well  founded."  That  this  constancy  is  not 
a  veritable  one  is,  I  think,  fully  shown  by  the  researches  of  the 
Vogts  as  well  as  by  the  results  of  the  present  study.  There  is  not 
the  degree  of  constancy  in  the  motor  response  which  the  earlier 
investigators,  as  for  example  those  which  are  cited  by  Sherrington, 
contended  there  is.  Moreover,  the  later  results  obtained  by  Sher- 
rington in  conjunction  with  Brown  which  have  been  discussed 
above  must  also  be  taken  into  account,  for  it  appears  to  me  they 
amply  demonstrate  the  opposite  of  that  which  Sherrington  wrote 
eight  years  ago,  and  prove  that  the  stimulation  of  the  same  cere- 
bral point  at  different  times  produces  varieties  of  action. 

Whether  or  not  the  variations  in  movement  associated  with 
cerebral  stimulation  are  to  be  correlated  with  normal  individual 
activities,  an  explanation  which  is  only  slightly  advanced  beyond 
that  of  the  Vogts,  is  a  question  for  the  solution  of  which  the 
closest  observations  and  correlations  of  the  normal  activities  and 
the  extents  and  the  variabilities  of  the  motor  cerebral  control 


VARIATIONS  IN  DISTRIBUTION  OF  MOTOR  CENTERS      147 

of  individual  animals  must  be  accumulated.  At  present  such  a 
view  appears  to  be  in  harmony  with  all  the  facts  which  are  known 
to  me  and  is  tentatively  put  forth  pending  further  studies.  It 
may  also  be  remarked  that  this  view,  in  a  special  form,  has  been 
suggested  by  Bolton  (2)  in  relation  to  his  anatomical  studies  of 
cases  of  "amentia"  and  of  "dementia,"  for  he  writes  that  the 
anatomical  variations  indicate  "the  likelihood  of  a  structural 
origin  for  individual  differences  in  mental  endowment,"10  and 
on  the  other  hand  he  says  the  histologically  differentiated  areas 
indicate  the  "limits  of  educability." 

The  results  of  the  present  research,  in  conjunction  with  the 
data  of  others  which  have  'been  recorded  above,  indicate  that  the 
connections  which  are  made  by  way  of  the  cortical  motor  cells 
are  not  definite  in  the  sense,  for  example,  that  there  is  a  passage 
of  an  impulse  from  a  Betz  cell  in  the  anatomically  defined  cere- 
bral motor  region  to  another  particular  efferent  cell  in  the  spinal 
cord,  but  that  the  connection  is,  in  special  senses  of  the  terms, 
promiscuous  or  irregular.  By  these  last  terms  I  mean  only  that 
the  connections  which  one  particular  efferent  or  afferant  cell 
makes  are  connections  with  a  great  number  of  neurones,  and  that 
the  impulses  resulting  .from  the  activity  of  a  cell  body  may  affect 
many  other  cells.  Or,  in  other  terms,  an  impulse  arising  in  one 
cell  may  activate  or  influence  only  one,  or  any  number,  of  the 
cells  which  are  anatomically  associated  with  the  particular  cell 
with  which  we  deal.  It  is  quite  generally  admitted  that  a  cer- 
tain cell  has  the  possibility  of  sending  its  impulses  along  the  main 
neuraxon  and  this  is  the  view  which  is  implicitly  apparent  in 
most  discussions  of  cerebral  function.  But  it  is  also  obvious 
that  since  this  neuraxon  gives  off,  as  it  passes  to  its  final  goal, 
certain  collaterals  it  is  quite  as  reasonable  and  quite  as  logical  to 
conclude  that  it  has  also  the  possibility  of  sending  impulses  along 
any  one  of  these,  or  along  the  main  neuraxon  and  any  number 

10  This  is  not  quoted  as  an  indication  of  sympathy  with  the  methods  and 
other  conclusions  of  Bolton,  who  has,  in  fact,  introduced  forms  of  expression 
in  regard  to  cerebral-mental  relations  which  are  obviously  grossly  inaccurate. 
Thus,  to  be  specific,  he  says  that  by  means  of  language  "it  is  possible  to  per- 
form the  highly  intricate  processes  of  cerebral  association,"  and  that  "if 
words  spontaneously  arise  in  a  cerebral  center,"  whatever  these  things  mean. 


148 


SHEPHERD  IVORY  FRANZ 


of  the  collaterals,  or  along  one  or  more  of  the  collaterals  to 
the  exclusion  of  the  main  trunk.  It  is  this  later  method  of 
looking  at  the  activities  of  the  cerebral  cells  which  appears  to  me 
to  solve  some  of  the  great  difficulties  of  the  exclusive  neuraxon 
activity  hypothesis. 

The  illustration  which  is  presented  here   (Figure  n)   is  one 


FIGURE  n.  Illustrating  the  conception  of  the  possibility  of  impulses  in  one 
cell  influencing  different  cells,  thus  resulting  in  different  reactions  or  differ- 
ent behavior.  The  primary  cells  A,  B,  and  C,  may  be  equally  well  considered 
to  be  afferent,  efferent,  or  associational.  Each  cell  may  also  be  considered 
as  a  simplification  of  a  group  of  cells. 


VARIATIONS  IN  DISTRIBUTION  OF  MOTOR  CENTERS      149 

which  gives  a  diagrammatic  conception  of  this  view.11  The 
activity  of  the  cell  B  in  its  discharge  may  be  represented  as 
passing  in  any  one  or  more  of  the  directions  taken  by  the 
branches  of  its  neuraxon.  Thus  the  activity  of  this  cell  may 
result  in  the  stimulation  of  the  cell  U,  or  the  cell  T,  or  the  cell 
V.  In  turn  the  activities  of  these  cells  (T,  U ,  F)  may  result  in 
the  stimulation  of  muscles.  On  the  other  hand  the  activity  of 
cell  A  may  through  its  collateral  acting  upon  cell  /  result  in  an 
activity  of  cell  T,  and  the  activity  of  cell  C  acting  through  its 
collateral  on  cell  N  may  also  bring  about  activity  in  cell  V .  Or, 
cell  C  acting  through  its  collateral  on  cell  G  may  activate  cell  B 
so  that  the  characteristic  B  activities  may  be  obtained.  In  turn 
the  activity  of  cell  B  may  influence  those  of  cells  A  and  C  almost 
directly,  thus  bringing  about  reactions  in  parts  which  are  nor- 
mally controlled  by  those  cells,  for  example  those  parts  connected 
with  cells  P  and  R,  and  those  with  cells  Y  and  Z,  When  it  is 
considered  that  cell  B  may  be  taken  as  a  representative  of  a  so- 
called  sensory  cell,  or  an  efferent  cell  which  is  normally  made 
active  when  it  received  the  effect  of  an  impulse  from  some 
other  cell  or  cells  in  other  portions  of  the  cerebrum,  for  example 
from  a  sensory  or  associational  center,  and  that  there  may  be  many 
of  these  which  exert  an  influence  upon  it,  it  will  be  realized  that 
the  behavior  resulting  from  the  activity  of  a  primary  receptive 
cell  may  greatly  vary  from  time  to  time. 

This  neurological  conception  may  be  applied  to  the  under- 
standing of  the  behavior  differences  of  individuals  and  also  of 
the  same  individual  from  time  to  time.  It  appears  probable 
that  the  variations  in  behavior  of  different  animals  and  of  the 
same  animal  at  different  times  to  the  same  form  of  stimulation 
are  dependent  upon  the  great  number  of  connections  and  upon  the 
variations  in  activity  which  the  connection  variations  make 
possible.  On  the  hypothesis  that  the  connection  between  cor- 
tical cells  is  definite,  in  the  sense  that  one  cell  acts  solely  or 

11  A  few  words  of  caution  may  be  said  regarding  the  diagram  and  its  ac- 
companying paragraph  of  text.  Neither  should  be  taken  too  literally,  neither 
should  be  considered  to  be  more  than  an  indication  of  a  possibility,  and 
neither  should  be  judged  without  the  preceding  and  the  following  context. 


150  SHEPHERD  IVORY  FRANZ 

principally  upon  one  other  cell,  we  shall  have  great  difficulty  in 
explaining  the  phenomena  in  man  or  in  animals  which  are 
grouped  under  the  general  heading  of  habit  formation.  To  show 
this,  let  us  briefly  consider  the  facts  regarding  the  formation 
of  habit  in  several  animals.  We  shall  then  realize  how  the  same 
stimulus  may  result  in  different  reactions  in  different  animals, 
and  how  in  one  animal  at  different  times  different  reactions  may 
result  from  the  same  stimulus.  Conversely  also  we  shall  get 
some  neurological  insight  into  the  possible  reason  for  similar 
reactions  in  different  animals  from  different  stimuli.  On  the 
assumption  of  definite  connections  and  definite  paths  of  dis- 
charge such  facts  are  neurologically  almost  unexplainable. 

L'et  us  take  for  consideration  a  young  cat,  four  to  six  months 
old,  since  an  animal  of  this  kind  is  readily  "educable."  If  the 
animal  is  hungry  it  will  be  better,  since  the  formation  of  the 
habit  is  then  more  readily  obtained  if  the  habit  has  one  of  its 
elements  concerned  with  the  obtaining  of  food.  We  prepare 
for  our  experiment  a  box  with  narrow  slats  in  the  front  and  a 
small  door  which  is  closed  with  a  bolt.  The  knob  of  the  bolt 
is  attached  to  a  cord  which  runs  along,  but  an  inch  under,  the 
top  of  the  box  and  which  the  animal  can  reach  either  with  its 
claws  or  by  arching  its  back,  or  by  biting  with  its  teeth.  When 
the  cord  is  pulled  downwards  or  pushed  upwards  or  moved  side- 
ways the  bolt  is  also  moved.  Any  one  of  these  actions  (there 
may  be  others  and  also  combinations  of  two  or  more  of  these 
actions)  will,  if  sufficiently  strong,  result  in  the  loosening  of 
the  bolt  which  keeps  the  door  closed,  and  when  the  door  is  thus 
opened  the  cat  is  enabled  to  escape  and  to  get  a  particle  of  food 
which  is  placed  outside. 

When  we  place  a  cat  in  an  enclosed  space  of  this  character 
there  is  a  very  decided  change  in  the  behavior  of  the  animal. 
It  usually  becomes  very  active.  This  activity  we  may  describe, 
in  terms  which  are  not  directly  scientific  in  their  psychological 
aspect,  as  being  due  to  the  desire  on  the  cat's  part  to  escape 
from  the  uncomfortable  situation  of  being  in  an  enclosed  place 
of  such  small  compass,  and  perhaps  partly  to  the  desire  for  the 
food  which  in  some  experiments  it  may  see  outside.  The  actions 


VARIATIONS  IN  DISTRIBUTION  OF  MOTOR  CENTERS      151 

of  the  particular  cat  under  these  conditions  are  about  the  same 
as  those  of  other  animals  of  the  same  species  which  are  placed 
in  such  a  situation.  The  animal  begins  to  scratch  at  the  front 
of  the  cage,  at  the  door,  at  the  sides,  at  the  top.  It  turns  here 
and  there,  it  takes  hold  of  everything  or  anything  which  it  can 
reach.  These  movements  are  not  performed  in  any  apparently 
logical  order  or  in  any  apparently  intelligent  manner  since  the 
animal  may  at  first  try  one  corner,  then  the  top,  perhaps  next 
the  door.  If  these  movements  do  not  result  in  the  escape 
of  the  animal  ;from  the  "unpleasant"  situation  the  cat  may 
remain  quiet  for  a  time  and  begin  all  over  again  scratching  at 
a  front  corner  or  a  back  corner,  trying  the  top,  the  door,  the 
slats  at  the  front.  Even  though  the  special  movements  do  not 
result  in  the  release  which  is  sought  the  movements  are  con- 
tinued, and  if  the  cat  tries  one  thing  and  does  not  escape  by 
so  doing  it  may  return  to  the  first  which  it  had  previously  found 
unsuccessful.  The  random  movements,  if  they  are  continued 
for  a  sufficiently  length  of  time,  eventually  result  in  the  animal's 
moving,  either  by  clawing  or  by  arching  its  back  or  by  biting, 
the  cord  which  holds  the  bolt.  When  the  bolt '  has  thus  been 
lifted  the  activities  of  the  animal  may  be  continued  for  some 
seconds  or  minutes  before  it  realizes  or  recognizes  that  the 
door  is  open  and  there  is  a  possibility  of  escape.  When  the 
animal  escapes  from  the  situation  it  finds  the  food  or  it  is  given 
a  small  piece  of  food.  When  it  is  returned  to  the  box  which  is 
again  bolted  it  goes  through  the  same  kinds  of  activities,  claw- 
ing here,  biting  there,  resting,  performing  movements  which  are 
apparently  purposeless  since  they  are  not  directed  to  the  part  of 
the  box  by  which  escape  becomes  possible,  or  towards  the 
mechanism  whereby  the  door  can  be  opened.  In  its  random 
movements  it  again  scratches  the  cord,  and  again  escapes  and 
gets  food.  At  the  next  trial  the  animal  goes  through  the  same 
sort  of  movements.  Finally  it  claws  the  cord,  gets  out,  and  in 
succeeding  tests  it  is  found  that  this  animal  which  at  first  escaped 
because  of  biting  the  cord  and  then  later  by  arching  its  back 
against  it,  and  again  by  clawing  at  the  cord  eventually  acquires 
the  habit  of  escape  by  utilizing  only  one  of  these  types  of  move- 


152  SHEPHERD  IVORY  FRANZ 

ment,  namely  the  scratching  or  clawing  at  the  cord.  Further- 
more it  is  found  that  when  an  animal  is  placed  in  this  situation 
it  eventually  acquires  the  habit  to  such  a  degree,  or  the  reaction 
is  facilitated  to  such  an  extent,  that  immediately  the  animal  is 
dropped  into  the  box  it  goes  to  the  particular  location,  claws  at 
the  cord  thus  opening  the  door,  escapes  and  obtains  the  food. 

Another  animal  goes  through  the  same  general  kinds  of  activi- 
ties in  its  escape  or  its  attempts  at  escape,  but  instead  of  acquir- 
ing the  habit  of  escaping  by  means  of  clawing  at  or  by  pulling 
the  cord,  it  acquires  the  habit  of  arching  its  back  and  rubbing 
against  the  cord,  thus  putting  the  cord  on  a  stretch  and  raising 
the  bolt.  A  third  animal  learns  to  escape  from  the  box  by  biting 
and  pulling  upon  the  cord. 

It  will  'be  observed  that  as  far  as  we  can  determine  all  three 
animals  have  been  stimulated  by  exactly  the  same  primary  forms 
of  stimuli.  They  have  been  stimulated  by  the  sight  of  the  box, 
by  the  appearance  of  the  slats  in  the  front,  by  the  closed  door, 
by  other  ill-defined  sensations  which  are  obtained  from  the  con- 
finement, perhaps  from  the  stimulation  of  a  variety  of  organs 
which  go  to  make  up,  in  human  perceptual  terms,  the  general 
feeling  of  being  enclosed  in  the  box.  The  sensory  elements 
which  are  present  in  these  three  cases  we  most  likely  have  the 
right  to  conclude  are  the  same.  The  emotional  elements  or  con- 
comitants we  do  not  know,  if  any  exist,  and  we  have  at  present 
no  means  of  determining  the  similarity  or  variety  of  these  mental 
conditions  if  they  exist.  It  is  to  be  noted  however  that  although 
the  sensory  stimuli  are  the  same  the  behavior  to  which  the  stimuli 
lead  differs  in  the  three  animals.  The  reactions,  it  will  be  ob- 
served, have  one  thing  in  common,  namely  that  they  result  in 
the  escape  of  the  animal.  The  actual  means,  however,  of  pro- 
ducing this  desired  situation  differs  for  the  three  animals. 
Neurologically  it  is  not  only  likely  but  it  is  almost  certain  that 
the  impulses  from  the  sense  areas,  those  so-called  associational 
impulses  which  start  from  the  cells  in  the  sensory  regions  of 
the  brain,  eventually  concentrate  in  these  three  animals  in  dif- 
ferent motor  areas,  or  to  put  the  matter  in  more  probable  terms, 
that  the  impulses  originating  in  similar  sensory  cells  in  all  three 


VARIATIONS  IN  DISTRIBUTION  OF  MOTOR  CENTERS      153 

animals  reach  (a)  the  same  or  (b)  a  different  frontal  lobe  cell 
or  group  of  cells  in  all  animals,  and  that  (a)  this  similar  frontal 
cell  or  group  of  cells  discharges  into  different  cells  in  the  pre- 
central  area,  or  that  (b)  the  different  frontal  cells  influence 
motor  cells. 

Now  it  will  furthermore  be  found  that  if  an  animal  which 
has  acquired  the  habit  of  escape  from  a  box  of  this  character, 
either  by  clawing  or  biting  or  rubbing  against  the  cord,  be 
placed  in  the  same  box  and  the  movement  which  it  has  been 
accustomed  to  make  results  in  no  food  or  in  no  release, 
this  movement  is  gradually  given  up.  The  situation  becomes 
different,  although  the  sensory  stimuli  remain  the  same.  By 
holding  the  bolt  or  by  making  some  external  change  in  the 
mechanism  (which  is  not  seen  by  the  animal)  to  prevent  the 
escape  by  any  movement  of  the  cord,  but  to  permit  the  escape 
whenever  the  animal  sits  quietly  and  licks  itself,  or  washes  its 
face  by  the  characteristic  series  of  paw  movements,  or  scratches 
itself,  the  animal  soon  gives  up  the  first  habit  which  it  had 
formed  and  replaces  it  by  behavior  which  in  itself  has  not 
apparently  any  direct  bearing  upon  the  desired  result.  We  then 
have  a  similar  primary  series  of  stimuli  which  at  one  time 
results  in  a  particular  mode  of  activity  (clawing  the  cord), 
and  at  another  time  in  a  different  mode  of  activity  (licking 
itself)  in  the  same  animal.  Both  lead  to  what  may  be  con- 

(sidered  the  desired  result,  namely  the  escape  from  the  enclosed 
box. 
It  should  be  understood  that  the  sensory  stimuli  in  two  ex- 
periments of  this  character  are  not  the  same  in  their  totality. 
The  initial  or  primary  sensory  stimuli  are,  however,  the  same. 
When  after  the  receipt  of  the  primary  , sensory  stimulation  a 
reaction  is  produced  the  reaction  results  in  an  additional  sensory 
stimulation,  and  this  secondary  stimulation,  or  the  combination 
of  the  secondary  with  the  primary,  may  give  rise  to  another 
reaction.  The  animal  which  claws  first  at  the  front  of  the 
cage  after  the  receipt  of  the  primary  stimulation  has  thereby 
a  character  or  combination  of  stimulation  different  from  that  of 
the  animal  which  first  reaches  for  the  top  of  the  cage  and  tries 


154  SHEPHERD  IVORY  FRANZ 

to  climb  out  in  that  way.  Each  animal  however  does  have 
the  same  primary  stimulation,  or  at  least  the  same  general 
primary  stimulation,  visual,  tactile,  organic,  etc.  To  go  back 
to  the  original  stimulation  we  may  even  wonder  why  such 
similar  primary  stimuli  have  produced  such  diverse  methods  of 
behavior  as  that  of  clawing  at  the  slats  at  the  front  of  the 
cage  and  that  of  trying  to  bite  the  slats  at  the  top.  In  either 
case,  whether  we  consider  the  primary  stimulus  or  the  collec- 
tion of  stimulations  which  make  up  the  whole  experience  of 
the  animal  in  the  box  the  sensory  stimulations  are  sufficiently 
alike  to  presuppose  (on  the  basis  of  exactness  of  neurological 
connections)  an  approximate  similarity  in  the  activity  of  the 
cerebral  sensory  areas,  and  to  suggest  (on  the  same  hypothesis) 
that  the  efferent  cerebral  activity  should  be  the  same.  This  is, 
of  course,  on  the  very  generally  accepted  belief  that  the  impulses 
from  corresponding  sensory  cells  will  always  go  to  correspond- 
ing efferent  cells. 

On  the  hypothesis  that  there  are  definite  connections  estab- 
lished by  means  of  certain  cerebral  neurones,  and  the  hypothesis 
that  when  the  stimulation  reaches  a  particular  sensory  center 
it  flows  into  other  areas,  eventually  reaching  the  motor  area 
and  resulting  in  a  particular  type  of  movement,  the  varying 
activities  of  these  animals  are  not  understandable.  It  is  not 
an  explanation  to  say  that  one  animal  has  certain  sensory  stimuli 
like  those  of  another,  but  that  there  are  different  activities. 
Neurologically,  there  must  be  a  basis  for  the  different  kinds  of 
behavior.  When  we  consider  the  possibility  that  the  discharge 
from  a  certain  cell  may  pass  not  only  along  the  main  neuraxon 
but  also  along  any  one  or  all  of  the  collaterals  and  that  in  this 
manner  we  have  the  neural  activity  diffused,  we  have  a  possible 
explanation  of  the  variety  of  the  actions  of  the  same  animal 
under  similar  conditions.  If  the  receiving  cell  were  definitely 
and  solely  (anatomically  and  physiologically)  connected  with 
a  special  cell  or  group  of  cells,  the  same  sensory  stimulus  should 
result  in  the  same  kind  of  reaction  in  different  animals  and  in 
the  same  animal  at  different  times.  But  we  find  that  at  first 
the  cat  makes  many  random  movements.  In  other  words,  neu- 


VARIATIONS  IN  DISTRIBUTION  OF  MOTOR  CENTERS      155 

rologically  we  are  led  to  conceive  that  the  discharge  of  the 
sensory  or  receptive  element  is  not  only  along  the  main 
neuraxon  but  is  along  all  of  the  collaterals  as  well,  and  each 
in  turn  acts  upon  its  cells  or  group  of  cells,  producing  impulses 
which  eventually  result  in  movements.  These  movements  are 
random,  i.e.,  not  directly  correlated  with  the  stimuli  nor  with 
the  desired  result,  but  as  the  experience  is  repeated  the  animal 
gives  up  all  but  a  certain  amount  of  the  reaction.  Its  behavior 
has  changed.  It  is  not  only  believable  but  probable  that  in 
the  development  of  a  particular  type  of  activity  or  in  the  pro- 
duction of  a  particular  association  or  habit,  such  as  that  of 
scratching  or  of  biting  or  of  arching  the  back,  we  may  have 
two  different  neurological  conditions.  To  use  the  simplified 
diagram  which  has  been  given  above  we  may  say  that  at  first 
the  discharge  takes  place  along  all  the  branches  of  the  neuraxon, 
but  this  diffuse  discharge  eventually  gives  place  to  a  discharge 
along  one  of  the  collaterals  or  along  the  main  branch.  The 
variation  in  behavior  of  two  animals  may  then  be  due  to  the 
primary  stimulation  of  corresponding  cells,  but  in  one  case  the 
habitual  reaction  is  determined  by  the  flow  of  the  impulses 
from  these  cells  along  the  course  of  the  main  neuraxon  and  in 
the  other  case  the  habitual  reaction  is  determined  by  the  passage 
df  the  impulse  along  a  collateral  These  impulses  reaching 
different  efferent  elements  produce  the  varieties  of  behavior. 

The  results  of  the  preceding  study  (5)  of  the  variation  in 
symptoms  accompanying  similar  cerebral  lesions  in  the  insane 
have  also  a  bearing  upon  the  present  work.  In  that  study  it 
is  shown  that  in  four  collections  of  cases  of  patients  suffering 
from  different  mental  diseases,  in  whose  brains  atrophies  of 
the  frontal  or  anterior  regions  of  the  cerebrum  were  detected 
at  autopsy,  there  is  no  apparent  relation  between  the  symptoms 
and  the  localization  or  the  degree  of  the  cerebral  damage.  On 
the  assumption  that  there  are  definite,  in  the  sense  of  singular 
and  similar,  functions  and  functional  connections  in  each  hemi- 
sphere in  all  individuals  such  divergencies  in  the  symptomatology 
are  not  readily  understandable. 

At  the  same  time  the  accounts  of  my  experiments  on  the 


I56  SHEPHERD  IVORY  FRANZ 

functions  of  the  frontal  lobes  (4)  contain  material  of  impor- 
tance for  the  understanding  of  the  cerebral  functional  relations. 
In  that  work  it  was  shown  that  after  an  animal  had  been  trained 
to  react  in  a  certain  way,  or  had  acquired  a  certain  habit,  the 
habit  was  lost  when  parts  of  the  frontal  lobes  were  Separated 
from  the  remainder  of  the  brain  or  when  they  were  destroyed. 
Even  after  the  loss  of  a  great  amount  of  the  frontal  regions 
such  an  animal  could,  however,  reacquire  the  lost  habit.  The 
reacquired  habit  could  again  be  destroyed  (or  lost)  if  addi- 
tional portions  of  the  frontal  lobes  were  extirpated,  and  in  some 
animals  it  was  possible  to  show  that  the  same  habit  could  be 
again  acquired. 

It  is  neither  satisfying  nor  sufficient  to  say  that  in  the  latter 
experiments  there  has  been  an  inhibition,  for  this  can  only 
give  to  the  facts  another  name.  Nor  does  it  suffice  to  say  that 
there  has  occurred  a  sort  of  "diaschisis,"  since  this  also  is  only 
another  means  of  expression  of  the  generalized  fact  of  loss 
of  function.  What  must  be  concluded  from  these  facts  is  that 
at  the  time  of  the  first  extirpation  there  was  a  "diaschisis,"  or 
blocking,  or  break,  in  the  normal  chain  of  cerebral  activity  (or 
neurologically  and  anatomically,  of  the  cerebral  connections). 
After  the  second  learning  of  a  habit  and  its  loss  subsequent 
to  a  second  and  more  extensive  extirpation,  "diaschisis"  may 
again  be  taken  as  the  explanation  of  the  fact.  Another  ex- 
planation beyond  those  of  "diaschisis"  or  inhibition  is  de- 
manded, however,  if  or  the  phenomena  of  learning  after  the 
first  extirpation  of  the  cerebral  area  through  or  by  which  learn- 
ing or  habit  formation  normally  is  possible.  It  is  obvious  that 
the  normal  (i.e.,  the  first)  paths  cannot  be  traversed  again, 
for  these  have  been  interrupted,  or  perhaps  abolished.  It  is 
obvious  that  new  paths  or  new  possibilities  of  connections  must 
be  available.  In  other  words  for  a  reasonable  explanation  we 
are  thrown  back  upon  the  assumption  that  the  paths  for  reac- 
tions are  not  the  simple  anatomical  unities  which  have  been 
commonly  believed  in  but  that  these  paths  are  diverse  and  that 
anatomically  as  well  as  physiologically  they  are  complex. 

If  the  neurological  path  for  the  formation  of  a  habit  is  a 


VARIATIONS  IN  DISTRIBUTION  OF  MOTOR  CENTERS      157 

fixity — from  a  certain  sensory  center  to  the  frontal  lobes  and 
thence  to  the  motor  cortex — a  break  at  any  portion  of  the 
path  (diaschisis,  if  you  will)  would  prevent  for  all  time  the 
reacquirement  of  the  lost  association.  That  there  is  no  such 
fixity  is  evident  from  the  fact  that  relearning  is  possible.  The 
explanation  of  the  fact  must,  I  think,  be  sought  in  another 
direction,  and  the  one  which  has  been  suggested  above  appears 
most  reasonable.  It  appears  probable  that  in  the  acquirement 
of  a  habit  certain  paths  are  traversed  and  that  they  have  a 
certain  fixity,  but  it  is  also  probably  true  that  these  paths  are 
not  the  only  ones  that  may  be  used  to  bring  about  the  desired 
connection  or  association  between  the  sensory  and  motor  end 
stations.  Most  probably  other  subsidiary  paths,  if  it  be  con- 
sidered necessary  or  advisable  to  differentiate  between  the  first 
path  and  other  paths,  or  relatively  subsidiary  tracts,  are  avail- 
able when  "diaschisis,"  or  inhibition,  or  other  similar  conditions 
supervene  to  prevent  the  normal  course  of  the  cerebral  impulses. 
The  conditions  of  variability  and  the  conditions  of  variation 
in  the  particular  responses  which  come  from  rather  definite 
sensory  stimulation  in  different  individuals  lead  us  to  a  better 
understanding  of  the  neurological  conditions  which  we  must 
believe  are  present  in  individual  cases.  It  is  not  sufficient  to 
say,  as  is  commonly  said,  that  past  experiences  determine  reac- 
tions, for  this  is  only  a  consideration  of  the  matter  from  the 
external  viewpoint.  It  gives  no  conception  of  the  neurological 
conditions  which  enter  into  the  matter.  At  present  I  think 
it  will  be  admitted  that  we  are  quite  ignorant  of  the  conditions 
which  result  in  the  selection  (not  necessarily  conscious  of 
course)  of  a  definite  path  in  the  nervous  system.  It  is  un- 
doubtedly true  that  certain  paths  are  fixed  in  the  sense  that 
one  neurone  has  fairly  direct  connections,  synaptic,  however, 
with  other  neurones  and  also  that  one  neurone  may  have  con- 
nections with  a  half-dozen  or  more  other  neurones.  Why  the 
stimulation  of  one  neurone  should  usually  give  rise  in  one  in- 
dividual to  a  particular  reaction  and  the  stimulation  of  what 
we  believe  to  be  a  corresponding  neurone  in  another  individual 
.  results  in  a  reaction  which  differs  somewhat  from  the  first,  we 


158  SHEPHERD  IVORY  FRANZ 

are  not  aware.  It  is,  however,  of  some  consequence  and  of 
some  importance  to  realize  that  there  are  greater  possibilities  of 
connections  than  have  hitherto  been  assumed  or  believed  in. 

Only  on  the  ground  of  the  assumption  of  variations  or  possi- 
bility of  variations  in  the  connections  or  in  the  patency  of 
collateral  and  main  tracts  may  we  understand  the  behavior 
phenomena  to  which  the  same  stimuli  give  rise  in  different  in- 
dividuals. Only  on  this  basis  can  we  understand  the  various 
activities  of  different  races  and  of  different  individuals.  The 
different  races  have,  it  is  well  known,  different  types  of  reac- 
tion. Anatomically  we  have  no  good  reason  to  believe  that  the 
neuronic  connections  differ  widely  in  different  races,  nor  anato- 
mically have  we  any  good  reason  to  believe  that  the  neuronic 
connections  in  different  individuals  of  the  same  race  or  of  the 
same  family  differ  very  widely.  It  is  apparent,  however,  that 
physiologically  these  connections  are  very  greatly  different  for 
the  activity  of  the  neurones  gives  rise  to  behavior  of  quite 
different  characters. 

Thus  far  we  have  been  considering  what  is  doubtless  the 
most  simple  neurological  system,  a  system  much  simpler  by  far 
than  that  which  is  active  in  the  production  of  any  form  of 
behavior  higher  than  that  of  a  reflex.  When  we  deal  with  a 
system  containing  more  than  the  two  elements,  afferent  and 
efferent,  or  receptor  and  effector,  the  complexities  of  connec- 
tions and  the  possibilities  of  variation  in  the  physiological 
connections  become  apparent. 

In  this  respect  the  cerebral  cortex,  or  the  cerebrum  as  a 
whole,  may  be  looked  at  as  a  very  labile  organ  because  of  the 
numerous  possibilities  of  connections  which  may  be  made.  One 
cell,  let  us  say,  may  have  close  connections  with  a  half-dozen 
or  a  dozen  other  cells,  and  the  activity  of  the  primary  cell  need 
not  always  be  through  all  the  branches.  There  is  a  possibility 
of  a  change  in  the  direction  of  the  impulse  within  the  neurone. 
Thus  at  one  time  the  main  effect  may  be  due  to  the  influence 
exerted  through  a  certain  collateral,  and  at  another  time  the 
effect  may  be  due  to  the  impulse  passing  through  the  main 
axon  or  through  a  second  collateral.  If  this  be  true,  it  helps 


VARIATIONS  IN  DISTRIBUTION  OF  MOTOR  CENTERS      159 

to  understand  why  there  is  a  possibility  of  change  in  reaction 
and  a  variability  of  reaction  in  the  same  individual  from  time 
to  time.  At  one  time  the  individual  may  have  a  discharge  from 
a  cortical  motor  cell  along  the  main  neuraxon  acting  upon  a 
definite  cell  located  in  a  definite  region  of  the  spinal  cord.  At 
another  time  the  discharge  may  take  place  not  only  along  the 
main  neuraxon,  but  along  one  or  more  of  the  collateral  branches, 
the  actions  resulting  from  the  impulses  passing  through  the 
collaterals  being  added  to  that  due  to  the  impulse  along  the 
main  fiber,  and  the  actions  along  these  collaterals  producing 
effects  on  other  cells  which  either  inhibit  or  alter  in  character 
the  actions  which  were  formerly  produced,  or  new  reactions 
may  entirely  replace  the  original  activity  by  an  activity  of  a 
very  different  character. 

Nor  does  it  appear  necessary  to  believe  that  once  a  path, 
by  way  of  the  main  trunk  or  by  one  of  the  collaterals,  has  been 
fixed  that  this  fixity  is  a  permanency.  There  may  be  a  greater 
tendency  to  use  this  particular  path  after  it  has  been  used  a 
number  of  times,  but  it  may  be  said  with  certainty  that  the 
impulse  may  under  suitable  conditions  traverse  any  one  or  all 
of  the  other  collateral  paths.  In  a  state  of  "mental  panic"  a 
man  acts  very  differently  to  a  particular  stimulus  than  at  other 
times.  His  actions  may  be  more  diffuse  or  they  may  be  the 
opposite  of  those  which  he  habitually  performs  at  normal  times. 
Thus,  the  sound's  of  a  rifle-shot  heard  at  two  different  times 
although  both  be  of  equal  intensity  may  give  rise  to  varying 
reactions.  Especially  when  there  is  an  affective  condition,  such 
as  fear  or  apprehension,  do  we  find  such  changes  taking  place 
in  the  reaction.12  Neurologically,  however,  it  is  not  satisfying 
to  say  that  the  emotional  condition  gives  the  "set"  to  the  dis- 
charge of  a  particular  cell,  or  that  it  directs  the  character  of 
the  discharge,  for  we  know  nothing  of  the  neurological  con- 
ditions which  give  rise  to  or  accompany  affective  states.  But, 

a  Particularly  those  of  diffusion.  Neurologically  perhaps  we  may  con- 
sider such  diffusion  to  be  due  to  the  passage  of  impulses  from  a  cell  not 
only  along  the  path  commonly  traversed,  but  along  all  the  collateral  paths 
as  well. 


160  SHEPHERD  IVORY  FRANZ 

should  we  admit  that  the  emotional  state  can  alter  the  character 
of  the  motor  response  due  to  such  a  simple  stimulus  as  that 
of  the  sound  of  a  rifle-shot,  we  are  admitting  at  the  same  time 
that  the  impulse  from  a  sensory  cell,  or  group  of  cells,  may 
pass  through  certain  paths  at  one  time  and  through  other  paths 
at  other  times.  Such  a  condition  may  also  be  well  illustrated 
by  a  difference  in  behavior  when  no  affective  state  intervenes 
to  alter  the  reaction  or  when  the  affective  state  remains  the 
same  with  the  presentation  of  the  stimulus  at  different  times. 
An  illustration  of  this  is  that  of  the  differences  of  speech,  which 
are  special  reactions  or  forms  of  behavior,  when  the  same  picture 
of  an  object  is  shown  at  different  times.  At  one  time  such  a 
stimulus  (the  picture  of  an  apple)  may  bring  forth  the  reac- 
tion "Apple,"  at  another  time  "Apfel"  and  at  a  third  time 
"Pomme" 

It  seems  most  likely  that  these  variations  in  activity  are  due 
to  physiological  variations  in  the  traversing  of  the  axon  or  the 
collaterals.  It  is  not  unlikely  that  as  conductors  the  axon  and 
the  collaterals  are  physiologically  equal,  that  they  may  be  utilized 
equally  well  or  equally  often  if  occasion  demands  it,  and  that 
the  definiteness  of  response  to  any  particular  stimulus  is  only 
a  relative  definiteness. 

In  considering  the  functions  of  the  cerebrum,  therefore,  we 
must  rid  ourselves  of  any  preconceived  notions  regarding  the 
fixity  or  definiteness  of  connections.  Fixity  or  definiteness  of 
an  anatomical  nature  there  undoubtedly  is,  but  this  fixity  or 
definiteness  is  on  the  physiological  side  a  multiplicity  of  fixities 
and  definitenesses.  One  cell  undoubtedly  communicates  with 
many  others,  and  while  this  is  an  anatomical  fixity  it  does  not 
result  in  a  physiological  definiteness  since  at  one  time  such  a 
cell  may  be  conceived  to  discharge  in  one  direction  along  one 
collateral  and  at  another  time  in  another  direction  along  another 
collateral.  At  present  we  may  not  have  sufficient  information 
to  guide  us  in  determining  the  reasons  for  the  discharge  in  this 
or  that  direction  but  the  facts  at  hand  indicate  that  discharges 
do  take  place  in  this  manner. 

Somewhat  similarly  we  must  explain  the  facts  of  differences 


in  symptoms  which  are  associated  with  similar  cerebral  lesions 
which  have  been  referred  to  in  a  preceding  paragraph.  If  we 
conclude  that  the  cerebral  paths  for  habits  (or  in  a  gross  phreno- 
logical sense,  for  mental  operations)  need  not  be  the  same  for 
all  individuals  such  symptomatological  dissimilarities  in  con- 
nection with  like  lesions  become  clearly  understandable.  If  all  in- 
dividuals do  not  use  the  same  limited  portions  for  the  same 
activities  (or  again  in  a  phrenological  vein,  for  the  same  mental 
processes)  the  destruction  of  similar  portions  of  the  cerebrum 
in  different  individuals  need  not  produce  the  same  symptoms. 
There  are  at  hand  sufficient  facts  in  clinical  neurological  litera- 
ture to  support  the  contention  that  similar  lesions  do  not  always 
produce  similar  clinical  symptoms  or  do  not  result  in  similar 
mental  alterations.  There  are  also  at  hand  sufficient  facts  to 
warrant  the  conclusion  that  dissimilar  lesions  may  produce 
similar  symptoms.  From  the  extreme  viewpoint  of  body-mind 
relations  (to  which,  however,  I  do  not  adhere)  such  facts  are 
sufficient  to  lead  to  the  conclusion  that  the  same  mental  opera- 
tions are  not  always  due  to  the  activities  of  the  same  parts 
of  the  brain.  From  a  more  conservative  standpoint  the  facts 
warrant  the  conclusion  that  the  same  forms  of  behavior  are 
not  always  due  to  the  activities  of  the  same  cerebral  cells.  That 
the  variability  in  the  functional  cerebral  connections  should  ever 
have  been  considered  doubtful  is  probably  due  to  the  phreno- 
logical views  which  have  influenced,  and  in  fact  pervaded,  all 
neurological  literature  for  many  years. 


REFERENCES 

1.  BOEKE,  J.    Die  Regenerationserscheinungen  bei  der  Vertei- 

lung   von   motorischen    und    rezeptorischen    Nervenfasern. 
PFLUGER'S  Arch.  f.  d.  ges.  Physiol.,  1913,  151,  57-64. 

2.  BOLTON,  J.   S.     The  Brain  in  Health  and  Disease.     New 

York:  Longmans,  Green,  1914.    Pp.  xiv  +  479- 

3.  BROWN,  T.  G.  &  SHERRINGTON,  C.  S.     On  the  Instability 

of  a  Motor  Point.   Proc.  Roy.  Sac.,  1912,  B85,  250-277. 

4.  FRANZ,   S.   I.     On  the  Functions  of  the  Cerebrum:     The 

Frontal  Lobes.    New  York :  Science  Press.  1907.     Pp.  64. 
(Arch,  of  Psychol.  No.  2.) 

5  FRANZ,  S.  I.  On  the  Functions  of  the  Cerebrum:  Symptom- 
atological  Differences  Associated  with  Similar  Cerebral 
Lesions  in  the  Insane.  Pp.  1-79  of  this  MONOGRAPH. 

6.  GRUNBAUM,  A.  S.  &  SHERRINGTON,  C.  S.     Observations  on 

the   Physiology  of  the  Cerebral   Cortex  of   Some  of  the 
Higher  Apes.    Proc.  Roy.  Soc.,  1901,  B69,  206-209. 

7.  KENNEDY,  R.    Experiments  on  the  Restoration  of  Paralyzed 

Muscles  by  Means  of  Nerve  Anastomosis.     Brit.  Med.  J., 
1911,2,  14-15.     (Abst.) 

8.  KENNEDY,  R.    Experiments  on  the  Restoration  of  Paralyzed 

Muscles  by  Means  of  Nerve  Anastomosis.    Proc.  Roy.  Soc., 
1911,  B84,  75-78. 

9.  OSBORNE,  W.  A.  &  KILVINGTON,  B.     Central  Nervous  Re- 

sponse to  Peripheral  Nervous  Distortion.    Brain,  1910,  33, 
261-265. 

10.  SHERRINGTON,  C.  S.    The  Integrative  Action  of  the  Nervous 
System.    New  York:  Scribner's,  1906.    Pp.  xvi  +  4ii. 

11.  VOGT,   C.   &  O.     Zur  Kenntnis  der  elektrisch   erregbaren 

Hirnrindengebiete.  /.  /.  Psychol.  u.  Neurol,  1907,  8,  277- 


Vol.  XIX  APRIL,  1915 

H,t  £  PSYCHOLOGICAL  REVIEW  PUBLICATIONS       flfcjfc  fl0>  g£ 


EDITED  BY 

JAMES  ROWLAND  ANGELL.  UNIVEKSITY  OF  CHICAGO 
HOWARD  C.  WARREN,  PRINCETON  UNIVERSITY   (Index) 

JOHN  B.  WATSON,  JOHNS  HOPKINS  UNIVERSITY   (Review)  and 
SHEPHERD  I,  FRANZ,  GOVT.  HOSP.  FOR  INSANE   (Bulletin) 

STUDIES  FROM  THE  PSYCHOLOGICAL   LABORA- 
TORY OF  HARVARD  UNIVERSITY 


The  Psycho-physiological  Effect  of 

the  Elements  of  Speech  in 

Relation  to  Poetry 

By 

ROBERT  CHENAULT  GIVLER,  PH.D. 

Instructor  in  Psychology  in  the  University  of  Washington 


PSYCHOLOGICAL  REVIEW  COMPANY 

PRINCETON,  N.  J. 
AND  LANCASTER,  PA. 

ACBNTS:    G.   E.  STECHERT  &  CO.,  LONDON   (2  Star  Yard,  Carey  St.,  W.  C.;, 
LEIPZIG  (Koenigstr.,  37) ;  PARIS  (16  rue  de  Conde) 


THE  PSYCHO-PHYSIOLOGICAL  EFFECT  OF  THE 

ELEMENTS  OF  SPEECH  IN  RELATION 

TO   POETRY 

The  purpose  of  this  investigation  is  to  determine  by  means 
of  the  expressive  method  the  effects  produced  by  the  speech 
elements  in  poetry  upon  both  the  motor  and  introspective  con- 
sciousness. 

Lying  on  the  borderland  between  Esthetics  and  Psychology, 
the  investigation  aims  to  throw  light  (i)  upon  the  so-called 
"tonal  theory  of  poetry,"  by  measuring  the  emotional  value  of 
the  sounds  in  poetic  language  without  reference  to  alliterative 
or  grammatical  devices,  and  (2)  to  discover  what  auditory, 
kinaesthetic  and  organic  sensations  are  aroused  by  the  play  of 
vocal  functions  in  meaningful  as  well  as  meaningless  collocations 
of  the  elements  of  language. 

The  material  is  presented  in  the  following  order : 

1.  A  statistical  determination  of  the  frequency  of  the  various 
speech  elements  in  English  poetry, 

2.  Experiments  upon  the  psycho-physiological  effect  of  such 
elements  combined  in  simple  relations, 

3.  Transmogrifications   of   English  poetry  to   determine   re- 
action to  the  bare  tonal  elements,  and 

4.  The  esthetic  and  psycho-physiological   question:     Is  the 
psycho-physiological  value  of  the  poetic  sum  equal  to  the  sum  of 
the  psycho-physiological  values  of  the  separately  contributing 
phonetic  elements? 

i.  SOUND  FREQUENCY  IN  ENGLISH  POETRY 

Prior  to  undertaking  the  experimental  work  in  the  laboratory, 
an  elaborate  statistical  record  was  made  by  the  writer  of  the 
percentage  of  frequency  of  the  various  letter  sounds  in  the  lead- 
ing English  poets  from  Sydney  to  Rossetti.  The  basis  for 
this  work  was  the  observation  of  very  striking  differences  in  the 
acoustic  and  kinaesthetic  sensations  aroused  by  the  audible 


2  ROBERT  CHENAULT  GIVLER 

reading  of  different  poets.  Especially  was  the  motor  pattern  and 
the  tonal  display  of  such  poets  as  Byron  and  Keats  noticed,  and 
upon  analysis  it  was  found  that  sound  frequency  was  one  element 
likely  to  contribute  to  the  differences  between  the  effects  which 
they  produced.  A  previous  study  of  vocal  music  to  provide  an 
increased  sensitivity  to  tonal  effects,  and  of  phonography  to 
hasten  the  sensori-motor  reactions  necessary  in  making  the  tabu- 
lations were  found  to  have  been  invaluable  psychological  instru- 
ments in  this  preliminary  work.  This  analysis  required  over  four 
years  and  involved  the  recording  of  over  540,000  tonal  elements ; 
46  phonetic  rubrics  were  employed,  and  where  doubt  was  enter- 
tained over  the  classification  of  the  elements  under  the  various 
rubrics,  recourse  was  had  to  general  poetic  usage  as  well  as  to 
the  special  idiosyncracies  of  the  individual  poets.  All  the  poets 
were  analysed  upon  the  basis  of  the  current  English  speech,  the 
Standard  Dictionary  being  used  as  the  criterion  for  pronuncia- 
tion. The  "foot-  and  -quantity"  system  was  employed  to  deter- 
mine the  accentuation,  and  both  accented  and  unaccented  sounds 
were  registered  in  the  tabulations.  Approximately  1,000  lines 
of  the  maturest  and  most  melodious  verse  of  each  of  the  poets 
were  examined.  The  result  of  this  work  is  here  summarized : 

I.  English  poets  usually  employ  about  10  accented  to  8  un- 
accented sounds.  Shelley,  Browning  and  Swinburne  are  the 
notable  exceptions,  each  of  which  gives  the  ratio  of  nearly  10  to 
10.  But  their  rhythmic  patterns  determine  much  of  this. 

II.     Greater  variations,  general  and  individual,  are  noticed  in 
the  use  of  the  accented  than  of  the  unaccented  sounds. 

III.  Tennyson  and  Swinburne  deviate  most  from  the  average 
use  of  the  sounds ;  Milton  is  nearest  the  average  of  all  the  poets 
examined. 

IV.  The  greatest  individual  variations  are  found  among  the 
most  used  accented  consonants  which  occur  in  the  following 
descending  order  of  frequency :     R,  N,  L,  T,  S,  D,  M,  etc.     But 
the  smallest  individual  variations  in  the  use  of  the  unaccented 
sounds  occur  among  those  of  the  greatest  frequency  of  use, 
namely:    u,  i,  a. 

Only  a  few  double  consonantal  rubrics  were  employed  besides 


ELEMENTS  OF  SPEECH  IN  RELATION  TO  POETRY  3 

the  familiar  Ch,  Wh  (Hw),  Ng,  Sh,  and  Zh;  such  double  sounds 
as  Bl,  Cr,  etc.,  were  split  and  tabulated  as  two  sounds.  While 
the  work  was  in  progress  Josselyn's  investigations  (see  Scripture, 
"Elements  of  Experimental  Phonetics,"  p.  501 )  came  to  hand,  in 
which  it  was  shown  that  a  double  consonant  was  simply  a  single 
one  strengthened  and  lengthened,  in  so  far  as  the  time  estimation 
of  rhythmic  syllables  was  concerned;  but  whether  the  double  or 
triple  consonantal  combinations  were  felt  as  fusions  or  as  addi- 
tions did  not  enter  in  as  a  standardizing  agent  in  the  above  work 
of  tabulation. 

Perhaps  the  most  striking  thing  in  the  whole  work  was  the  con 
stant  observation  of  the  modifying  influence  of  R.  Besides 
being  the  most  used  sound  in  English  poetry,  it  is  the  one  most 
frequently  observed  as  modifying  the  quality  of  juxtaposed 
vowels  and  consonants,  and  when  so  found,  its  local  signature  is 
absorbed  by  them. 

2.     THE  PSYCHO-PHYSIOLOGICAL  EFFECT  OF 
SIMPLE  SOUNDS 

The  experimental  work  was  carried  on  in  the  Psychological 
Laboratory  of  Harvard  University  from  1911  to  1914.  It  began 
with  the  audible  recitation  of  groups  of  five  iambics,  such  as 
la-mo,  la-bo,  and  la-do.  The  O  was  long,  and  the  A  was  given 
as  the  'Italian'  A.  On  account  of  the  neglect  it  received  in 
the  arsus,  however,  it  became  the  neutral  vowel.  The  time  of 
the  recitation  was  taken  and  the  chief  results  were:  the  appear- 
ance of  a  caesura,  the  feeling  of  satisfyingness  at  the  fifth  iam- 
bic, the  changes  in  feeling-tone,  sensations  and  imagery,  as  the 
different  combinations  were  presented.  Only  the  above  three 
iambics  were  employed  this  way ;  in  order  to  obtain  a  record  that 
was  valuable  for  the  correlation  of  so  motor  a  function  as  speech, 
some  graphic  record  had  to  be  employed. 

HISTORICAL 

Briicke1  had  obtained  kymograph  records  of  ictus  and  arsis 

by  means  of  a  quill  marker  while  various  kinds  of  verse  were 

1  Briicke,  "Die  physiologischen  Grundlagen  der  neuhochdeutschen  Kunst." 


4  ROBERT  CHENAULT  GIVLER 

recited.2  Lip  movements  were  also  recorded  by  means  of  a 
lever  passing  from  the  lips  to  the  kymograph.  His  findings  were 
that  the  time  taken  to  tap  various  kinds  of  poetic  feet  were  almost 
equal;  at  least  the  arses  recurred  at  equal  periods  of  time,  and 
the  abruptness  of  the  departure  of  the  kymographic  tracings 
from  the  abcissa  line  was  found  to  vary  considerably  for  dif- 
ferent poetic  metres. 

Bourdon3  had  traced  the  neck  vibrations  at  a  particularly 
mobile  place,  while  certain  sounds  were  uttered,  and  found  very 
great  amplitudes  for  the  vowel  I,  less  great  for  O,  and  lesser 
still  for  A.  It  appears,  however,  that  the  average  amplitudes  for 
combinations  of  consonants  with  O  was  greater  than  those  with 
I  and  A ;  the  I-combinations  being  quite  the  lowest. 

In  regard  to  what  one  might  expect  with  reference  to  the 
general  permeability  of  the  psycho-motor  organism  to  stirs  of 
various  sorts,  Angell4  held  that  "only  those  sensations  breaking 
in  upon  a  state  of  relative  quiet  disturbed  the  psycho-physical 
mechanism  enough  to  make  any  peripheral  difference." 

Fere's  opinion5  on  this  matter  is  that  a  momentary  intel- 
lectual activity  is  accompanied  by  a  momentary  increase  in  power 
of  the  voluntary  muscles.  He  also  found  that  both  under  the 
influence  of  an  intellectual  effort  and  of  other  things  (e.g .,  speech 
and  odors)  certain  excitations  of  the  muscular  sense  were  aroused. 
His  studies  showed  that  during  the  exercise  of  speech  the  move- 
ments of  the  right  hand  were  influenced,  i.e.,  augmented,  just 
as  one  works  a  treadle  with  the  foot  and  finds  the  synchronous 
hand  movements  augmented  as  much  as  a  6th  or  a  5th ;  it  is  even 
stated  that  the  right  hand  in  gesture  plays  a  veritable  "esthesio- 
genic"  role.  But  the  correlation  of  the  amount  of  energy  ex- 
pended with  pleasurable  or  unpleasurable  states,  Fere  does  not 
report  in  the  case  of  speech ;  in  connection  with  odors  and  the 

"The  study  was  not  of  the  pure  iambic  or  other  line;  inversions  of  feet 
occurred,  as  usual. 

'Bourdon,  P.,  "L'application  de  la  methode  graphique  a  1'etude  de  1'inten- 
sitie  de  la  voix,"  L'Anee  Psychologique,  1897. 

4  Angell,  J.  R.,  "Organic  processes  and  consciousness,"  Psych.  Rev.,  1890. 

*Fere,  Ch.,  "Sensation  et  Mouvement,"  1900,  esp.  Chap.  3. 


like,  the  greater  energy  seems  to  be  aroused  in  a  state  of  pleasure. 
But  the  final  generalization  is  in  these  terms:  "La  sensation  de 
plaisir  se  resont  done  dans  une  sensation  de  puissance;  la  sensa- 
tion de  deplaisir  dans  une  sensation  d'impuissance." 

The  development  of  the  expressive  method  itself  is  a  fitting 
corollary  to  the  "modern  tendency  to  understand  all  conscious- 
ness in  motor  terms,"  and  to  connect  it  with  the  "motor  rather 
than  the  sensory  side  of  the  organism."6  Professor  Miinster- 
berg's  action  theory  allies  itself  with  the  same  tendency.  Such 
writers  as  Dearborn,7  Pillsbury,8  Alexander,9  and  others  give  a 
large  place  to  the  psycho-motor  side  of  the  neural  arc,  though 
treating  the  consciously  volitional  side  of  consciousness  with 
varying  degrees  of  prominence. 

In  their  experiments  upon  the  "Time  relations  of  poetic 
metres,"10  Hurst  and  Mackay  appear  to  have  justified  somewhat 
the  method  of  poetic  analysis  used  herein  (only  indirectly,  how- 
ever), and  while  their  subjects  only  scanned  silently  or  tapped 
empty  rhythms,  they  found  that  the  iambic  foot  was  really  short- 
long  and  thus  that  the  tapping  of  the  metrical  unit  laid  a  stress 
upon  the  so-called  accented  syllable.  Inasmuch  as  greater  differ- 
ences appear  among  the  poets  (as  found  by  the  tables  previously 
mentioned)  in  the  use  of  accented  than  of  unaccented  sounds,  the 
above  results  are  pertinent  to  the  present  investigation.  Fur- 
ther comparisons  of  method  and  results  are  hardly  possible;  they 
used  the  iambic  foot  only  in  octosyllabic  lines,  (e.g.,  Scott's 
poems),  and  even  then  the  iambus  was  frequently  exchanged  for 
other  kinds  of  feet. 

Scripture's  subjects11  read  rather  than  scanned  poetry.  Trip- 
let and  Sand  ford12  found  that  the  explosive  consonants  were 

"  Kostyleff,  quoted  from  E.  B.  Delabarre,  "Volition  and  motor  conscious- 
ness-theory," Psych.  Bull.,  1912. 

7  Dearborn,  G.  V.  N.,  "The  relation  of  muscular  activity  to  the  mental 
process,"  Am.  Ed.  Rev.,  1909  (14)  18. 

*"The  place  of  movement  in  consciousness,"  Psych.  Rev.,  1911,  (18)  83-99. 

'  Brit.  Jouxr.  of  Psychv  11911    (4)  pp.  239-67. 

10  Univ.  of  Toronto  Studies,  No.  3,  1899. 

11  Scripture,  E.  W.,  Yale  Psych.  Stud.,  Vol.  VII,  1899. 
""Studies  of  rhythm  and  meter,"  Am.  Jour,  of  Psych.,  XII,  1901. 


6  ROBERT  CHENAULT  GIVLER 

more  nearly  tapped  and  spoken  at  identical  times  than  were  the 
others.  S  was  almost  always  syncoped.  But  in  the  general,  they 
found  that  finger  stress  indicates  quite  well  the  vocal  stress, 
though  minuter  correlations  are  not  indicated.  To  the  above 
results  we  ally  those  of  Meumann13  which  state  that  the  time 
limit  of  syncope  is  but  0.02  seconds.  Miyake14  found  that  the 
beat  of  the  finger  came  before  the  beginning  of  the  vowel  when  it 
stood  alone,  when  it  had  a  glottal  catch,  when  it  was  short  or 
long,  followed  by  a  final  consonant,  or  when  it  was  short  or  long 
between  two  consonants.  Also,  except  in  the  case  of  B,  D,  and  G, 
the  beat  as  tapped  came  before  the  vowel  following  these 
consonants. 

With  regard  to  the  matter  of  correlating  qualitative  conscious- 
ness states  with  the  motor  consciousness,  there  is  to  be  men- 
tioned Dressler's  work15  where  increased  central  activity  seemed 
to  favor  increased  rapidity  in  voluntary  movements;  also  the 
work  of  Drozynski16  which  does  not  crystallize  into  any  specific 
positive  correlation,  but  shows  apparently  that  the  unpleasant 
stimuli  gave  the  more  noticeable  arousals.  But  by  'unpleasant' 
we  must  understand  here  the  many  meanings  of  the  term  in  the 
sense  of  Wundt's  tridimensional  theory.  This  writer  used  no 
iambics.17 

So  much  for  a  general  account  of  some  of  the  more  important 
and  resultful  experimentations  upon  the  motor  and  introspective 
phases  of  an  expressive  method  in  psychological  esthetics  (espe- 
cially in  re,  poetry).  But  to  come  down  to  the  particular 
elements  of  our  own  research,  especially  the  form  of  the  rhyth- 
mical presentation  and  the  apparatus  used;  and  first  the  rhythm 
form. 

"  "Untersuchtmgen  zur  Psych,  und  Aesth.  der  Rhythmus,"  Wundt's  Studien, 
X,  1894,  p.  419. 

14  See  Scripture,  "Elements  of  Experimental  Phonetics,"  esp.  Chap.  37. 

"  "Excitement  and  tapping  rates,"  Am.  Jour,  of  Psych.,  1891,  IV,  p.  523. 

""Atmungs  und  Pulssymptome  rhythmischer  Gefiihle,"  Wundt's  Psych. 
Stud.,  Vol.  7,  pp.  83-140. 

1T  See  also  for  the  effects  of  pleasant  and  unpleasant  music,  F.  Rehwoldt, 
"Ueber  respiratorische  Affectssymptome,"  W-undfs  Stud.,  Vol.  3,  pp.  149-192. 


ELEMENTS  OF  SPEECH  IN  RELATION  TO  POETRY  ^ 

THE  CHOICE  OF  THE  IAMBIC  FOOT 

It  was  shown  above  that  iambus  is  a  sufficiently  character- 
istic form  to  be  used  as  a  vehicle  for  sounds  (op.  tit.  Hurst  and 
Mackay).  There  is  another  justification,  however.  From  the 
writer's  own  experience  in  the  field  of  literature,  the  standard, 
as  well  as  the  most  dignified  line  of  English  poetry  is  the  iambic, 
decasyllabic  line;  the  comic  finds  a  place  much  more  readily  in 
the  octosyllabic  (and  trochaic)  line,  or  in  still  smaller  forms. 
No  longer  line  than  this  has  succeeded  for  great  lengths  of 
verse,  and  most  of  the  sustained  work  of  any  considerable  length 
(barring  of  course  Coleridge's  "Lyrical  Ballads")  is  written  in  it, 
and  even  lighter  works  such  as  sonnets  are  not  rendered  over 
heavy  by  its  use.  The  iambic  foot  was  chosen  because  it  appeared 
to  be  the  standard  foot  in  English  poetry,  not  because  exceedingly 
frequent  inversions  of  it  did  not  occur,  nor  because  dactylic  and 
anapestic  innovations  were  not  part  of  the  very  body  of  even  the 
heavier  epics,  nor  because  runover  lines  did  not  frequently  ren- 
der the  iambic-trochaic  mele  of  feet  difficult  of  interpretation 
in  favor  of  one  or  the  other  kinds  as  the  predominant  foot,— but 
because  the  stress  of  the  accented  syllable  of  the  iambus  seemed 
to  bring  more  into  prominence  the  sounds  meant  to  be  stressed 
than  did  that  of  the  trochee.  Hurst  and  Mackay  (op  tit.}  found 
indeed  that  the  iambus  detained  its  ictus  in  the  motor  conscious- 
ness twice  as  long  as  its  arsis,  while  the  time  relation  of  ictus  and 
arsis  in  the  trochee  was  only  3/2  to  i . 

The  experiments  carried  on  by  Stetson18  and  Bingham19  had 
effectually  shown  the  advisability  of  employing  some  simple  vol- 
untary process  as  a  basis  for  psycho-motor  correlation.  The 
method  herein  employed  is  practically  the  same  as  theirs.  The 
voluntary  process  used  was  the  tapping  movement  of  the  right 
index  finger.  This  movement  is  exceedingly  simple  and  natural, 
and  soon  tends  toward  automatism,  leaving  one's  attention  entirely 
free  to  be  directed  upon  the  stimulus.  Very  rarely  did  the 
finger  movement  return  to  consciousness  after  it  had  become 
automatic ;  when  it  did  so,  introspection  showed  a  very  unpleasant 

18  "Rhythm  and  Rhyme,"  Harvard  Psych.  Stud.,  Vol.  I. 
"  "Studies  in  Melody,"  Harvard  Psych.  Stud.,  Vol.  II. 


8  ROBERT  CHENAULT  GIVLER 

and  turgid  state  of  consciousness ;  which  state  seems  to  be  present 
usually  when  any  chain  of  habitual  responses  is  broken. 

THE  APPARATUS 

The  form  of  the  apparatus  was  as  follows :  Upon  two  tables 
placed  about  two  metres  apart,  revolving  brass  drums  were  fas- 
tened ;  over  these  drums  passed  a  smoked  paper  belt ;  the  driving 
mechanism  was  at  one  end,  the  record-taking  device  was  at  the 
other.  The  subject  sat  comfortably  at  the  side  of  the  table  and 
laid  his  right  arm  on  a  flat  wooden  rest  having  a  notch  sufficiently 
long  to  avoid  all  possible  interference  with  the  index  finger, 
which  was  left  free  to  move  throughout  its  entire  natural  range 
of  flexion  or  extension.  To  have  had  the  finger  strike  against 
some  resisting  surface  would  have  prevented  our  detecting  any 
slight  variations  which  the  stimuli  produced  and  inasmuch,  also,, 
as  the  characteristic  departures  of  the  tapped  strokes  from  the 
abscissa  line  were  of  great  importance  for  most  of  the  subjects, 
the  lack  of  objective  controls  in  the  tapping  was  an  obvious 
advantage. 

The  periodic  movement  of  the  finger  was  recorded  as  follows : 
the  end  of  the  finger  was  placed  in  an  oilcloth  cot  which  was 
used  for  all  the  experimental  work  without  being  changed,  and 
from  the  cot  ran  a  fine  silk  thread  up  over  a  small  brass  pulley 
(always  kept  well  oiled)  through  a  guide,  and  was  fastened  to  a 
small  aluminum  marker  of  triangular  shape.  From  the  other  end 
of  the  marker  ran  a  small  rubber  band  to  an  unright  support. 
The  point  of  the  marker  rested  on  the  smoked  ribbon,  at  a  place 
on  its  surface  quite  close  to  the  vertical  axis  of  the  drum,  and 
so  neatly  was  this  whole  apparatus  constructed  and  so  slight 
was  the  tention  of  the  rubber  band,  that  it  was  hardly  per- 
ceptible to  the  subjects  and  did  not  interfere  with  the  freedom 
and  naturalness  of  the  movement.  The  tension  was  not  altered 
throughout  the  experiments.  The  thread  and  rubber  band  were 
renewed  in  duplicate  at  about  equal  intervals  and  thus  the  mechan- 
ical errors  in  the  recording  device  were  reduced  to  a  minimum. 

The  smoked  paper  ribbon  was  driven  by  a  gravity  motor  of 
sufficiently  constant  speed  to  reduce  the  error  of  inconstancy  to 


ELEMENTS  OF  SPEECH  IN  RELATION  TO  POETRY  9 

less  than  i  per  cent.  Its  rate  was  i  cm.  =  1.54  sec.  The  driving 
mechanism  was  enclosed  in  a  i>6  in.  soft-pine  box,  lined  with 
very  heavy  felt,  and  the  only  sound  audible  was  an  exceedingly 
faint,  and  not  unpleasant  whirr,  which  soon  became  accommo- 
dated and  was  never  again  noticed.  A  control  string  passed  from 
the  motor  up  over  a  pulley  to  the  other  end  of  the  belt  to  where 
the  experimenter  sat,  and  thus  the  movements  of  the  experimenter 
were  very  slight. 

As  the  finger  moved  up  and  down  while  the  ribbon  revolved, 
tracings  were  made  on  the  smoked  surface  and,  since  the  pointer 
accurately  recorded  the  full  extent  of  finger  movement  as  well  as 
such  qualitative  differences  as  suddenness  and  quiverings  in 
flexion  and  extension,  the  smoked  paper  ribbon  translated  much 
of  the  voluntary  movement  into  visible  terms. 

No  suggestions  or  illustrations  were  ever  given  as  to  rate  or 
extent  of  finger  movement;  each  subject  was  allowed  to  make  his 
own  pattern,  and  for  this  a  little  preliminary  tapping  was  em- 
ployed using  empty  5-iambic  lines.  In  recording  the  introspec- 
tion, which  was  done  without  inserting  a  screen  between  subject 
and  experimenter,  great  care  was  always  taken  to  betray  no  sign 
that  the  introspection  given  was  agreeable  or  not  to  the  results 
sought  after.  At  least,  all  conscious  control  of  the  matter  was 
assiduously  avoided. 

The  apparatus  was  arranged  so  that  the  subject  sat  facing  the 
window,  from  which  only  a  patch  of  sky  was  visible;  the  aspect 
was  northerly,  and  there  being  little  or  no  direct  sunlight,  the 
lighting  of  the  room  was  fairly  constant  throughout  the  whole 
period  of  experimentation. 

PRELIMINARY  EXPERIMENTS 

The  first  experiment  to  be  tried  with  the  above  tapping  device 
was  a  decasyllabic  line  made  of  five  iambics,  repeating  la-mo. 
This  line  was  repeated  five  times.  The  instructions  ran  as  fol- 
lows: "This  is  an  experiment  upon  the  psycho-motor  effect  of 
the  sounds  in  poetry;  while  you  recite  the  line,  tap  at  each  ac- 
cented syllable ;  take  your  own  time  to  do  it,  tap  in  a  natural  way, 
in  as  long  or  as  short  strokes  as  you  please ;  say  it  in  a  clear  voice 


io  ROBERT  CHENAULT  GIVLER 

and  then  introspect  upon  the  three  factors  of  feeling-tone,  sensa- 
tions and  imagery,  if  all  three  come;  otherwise  just  give  me 
the  introspectional  conscious  content,  much  or  little;  it  is  the 
sounds  and  their  effects  which  you  are  to  attend  to.  I  pull  this 
string  and  start  the  motor;  after  that,  whenever  you  are  ready, 
recite  and  tap;  the  line  is  to  be  spoken  and  tapped  five  times; 
pause  between  the  lines  just  enough  to  control  the  start  of  the 
next  line ;  have  you  got  the  instructions  in  mind  ?  are  you  ready  ? 
etc."  This  instruction  was  not  repeated  in  toto  at  every  hour's 
work  to  every  subject;  as  much  of  it,  however,  as  was  deemed 
necessary  from  psychognostic  reasons  was  repeated,  in  order 
to  get  the  same  'set'  for  each  group  of  experiments.  Inasmuch 
as  the  motor  field  was  so  narrow,  the  tapping  soon  became 
automatic,  and  the  instructions  could  be  reduced  to:  "This  is  to 
be  tapped  as  the  others  were, — on  the  accented  syllable."  And 
since  most  of  the  experiments  were  written  out  and  the  accents 
marked  in  red,  this  fact  rendered  full  instructions  obsolete. 

La-mo  was  followed  on  the  same  day  by  two  other  experi- 
ments, la-bo  and  la-ro.  All  of  the  eight  subjects  found  the 
la-ro  pleasant;  one  subject,  W.,  found  la-mo  unpleasant,  and 
A.  found  la-bo  unpleasant.  In  general,  la-mo  was  found  to  have 
a  "soft,  smooth  character,"  like  the  gentler  sounds  of  nature; 
la-ro,  on  the  other  hand  was  said  to  represent  the  roar  of  waves 
and  to  have  less  personal  reference  than  la-mo;  while  la-bo  im- 
plied something  insistent  and  was  referred  to  as  "trivial." 

One  cannot  lay  much  importance  upon  the  affect-motor  corre- 
lations in  these  experiments,  for  the  subjects  had  not  yet  become 
accustomed  to  the  tapping;  A.  and  W.,  for  example,  found  it 
more  convenient  and  natural  (?)  at  first  to  represent  the  ictus 
by  an  up-stroke  of  the  finger.  The  down  stroke  was 
suggested,  and  they  attempted  it,  but  for  the  first  few  weeks, 
at  least,  found  that  it  was  more  difficult  to  employ  it.  The 
records,  nevertheless,  were  measured  with  respect  to  the  ictus, 
whether  it  had  been  functioned  by  an  up  or  a  down  stroke.  It 
seemed  very  curious  that  an  accented  syllable  should  be  repre- 
sented by  means  of  the  weaker  of  the  two  movements  of  the 
finger. 


ELEMENTS  OF  SPEECH  IN  RELATION  TO  POETRY         11 

Of  these  three  combinations,  la-bo  appears  to  have  aroused 
the  greatest  feeling  of  energy.  A.  seemed  to  find  it  so;  B.  cer- 
tainly declared  it  as  such;  L.  and  T.  also  indicated  the  same 
tendency.  But  B.  moved  his  finger  farther  in  the  recitation 
of  la-mo  and  la-ro  than  he  did  in  the  more  "energetic"  la-bo. 
So  did  L.,  while  T.  who  found  la-ro  to  represent  "something 
substantial"  employed  the  greatest  force  for  that  sound,  and 
was  consistent  with  respect  to  la-bo,  which  he  called  more  active 
than  la-mo.  But  this  was  all  in  the  learning  stage,  and  it  is 
not  surprising  to  find  that  practice  increases  the  length  of  the 
tappings,  on  account  of  the  greater  familiarity  and  confidence 
with  the  work  which  it  brings.  The  subjects  were  asked  to 
rank  these  three  experiments  according  to  pleasantness,  and 
the  following  scheme  shows  what  relation  degrees  of  pleasure 
have  to  motor  discharge  in  this  first  group  of  experiments, 
(Descending  pleasantness  represented  by  A.  B.  C.) 

Subject  (A  Tapp.)  (B.  Tapp.)  (C.  Tapp.) 

Av.                             Av.  Av. 

A -ro  44.1  mm.  -mo  38.3  mm.  -bo    44.9  mm. 

B -ro  78.1  -mo  83.5  4>o    76.2 

F -ro  45.8  -mo  58.0  -bo    46.4 

L -ro  51.8  -mo  66.7  -bo    50.7 

N -ro  86.8  -bo    87.7  -mo  83.3 

T -ro  76.0  4)0    73.6  -mo  62.6 

W -ro  54.8  -mo  24.6  -bo    55.3 

Z -ro  43.5  -mo  51.2  -bo    42.3 

Three  of  the  subjects,  A.,  W.,  and  Z.,  ascend  in  length  of 
tappings  as  the  feeling  tone  ascends.  Three  of  them,  B.,  L., 
and  T.,  all  give  the  medium  stroke  to  the  experiment  they  found 
in  the  middle  degree  of  pleasantness  but  all  three  also  ally  the 
greatest  degree  of  pleasure  to  the  least  amount  of  motor  dis- 
charge. The  other  two,  F.,  and  N.,  show  no  correlation  at  all. 

The  next  two  experiments  were  the  combinations  de-ho,  and 
ho-de  (both  long  vowels).  From  graphing  the  objective  re- 
sults it  appeared  that  ho-de  produced  on  the  average  a  greater 
motor  output  than  did  de-ho.  Also  the  curve  of  the  latter  rises 
and  falls, — from  the  first  to  the  middle  a  rise,  and  from  the 
middle  to  the  end  a  descent;  this  was  general  for  all  the  sub- 
jects: some  reported  a  little  exhilaration,  strain  and  the  like, 
but  no  feeling  of  fatigue,  or  exhaustion.  The  ho-de  curve. 


12  ROBERT  CHENAULT  GIVLER 

on  the  other  hand,  rises  almost  continuously  from  start  to  finish, 
with  a  remarkable  rise  on  the  fourth  foot  of  the  fifth  group, 
and  a  no  less  striking  descent  on  the  last  accented  syllable  of 
the  series.  But  the  last  three  groups  show  the  same  general 
tendency, — that  of  emphasizing  the  motor  prominence  of  the 
fourth  foot  of  the  group.  The  first  group  of  either,  however, 
shows  almost  the  same  kind  of  form,  which  may  be  due  to  the 
persistence  of  the  motor  "set." 

Three  of  the  subjects,  A.,  L.,  and  T.  preferred  ho-de;  in 
each  case  the  tapped  strokes  were  longer  for  the  more  pleasant; 
but  in  the  former  experiments,  only  one  of  them,  A.,  showed 
this  feature.  All  the  other  subjects,  B.,  F.,  N.,  W.,  and  Z. 
manifested  a  preference  for  de-ho;  all  but  N,  as  mentioned  above, 
tapped  shorter  strokes  while  reciting  it.  Four  of  the  subjects 
found  the  vocal  construction  caused  by  the  "-de"  an  unpleasant 
feature.  But  inasmuch  as  there  was  no  objective  standard  of 
intensity  or  other  vocal  quale  which  was  to  be  followed,  the 
matter  of  constriction  cannot  be  raised  to  a  very  high  impor- 
tance. One  can  say  "ho-de"  with  countless  degrees  of  energy 
and  the  like,  and  usually  no  subject  intensified  an  unpleasant 
sensation;  rather  was  the  voice  weakened  and  lowered  to  avoid 
it.  On  the  same  day,  also  as  de-ho  and  ho-de  were  given,  the 
combination  ra-fo  (both  vowels  long)  was  given.  The  explosive 
character  of  the  f  tended  upon  repetition  to  destroy  the  pleasant- 
ness with  which  it  started  out. 

The  graphings  showed  a  remarkable  steadiness  of  motor  reac- 
tion for  this  combination  until  the  last  group  of  five  iambics 
was  reached. 

The  next  two  experiments  were  de-sto,  and  sto-de  (vowels 
both  long).  Curiously  enough,  the  differences  in  the  amount 
of  motor  discharge  did  not  appear  until  the  fourth  and  fifth 
groups,  and  while  the  ho-de  graph  kept  rising  after  the  third 
group,  and  de-ho  fell,  here  the  case  was  altered  completely; 
de-sto  showed  an  ascent,  but  in  the  middle  of  the  line  only  ( !)  ; 
but  again,  the  accented  O  produced  a  slower  reaction  than  did 
the  E.  One  must  remember,  of  course,  that  not  only  is  the 
accented  syllable  different  in  each  of  these  four  experiments, 


ELEMENTS  OF  SPEECH  IN  RELATION  TO  POETRY          13 

but  also  is  the  unaccented  syllable.  Furthermore,  the  subjects 
reported  that  not  only  did  the  iambic  attempt  to  become  a  trochee, 
but  the  unaccented  syllable  also  tended  to  demand  an  accent! 
Correlating  the  feeling-tone  with  the  motor  discharge  of 
these  two  experiments,  we  find  that  subjects  A.  and  F.  tapped 
longer  strokes  for  the  pleasant  than  for  the  unpleasant  experi- 
ments; N.,  W.,  and  Z.  reversed  this;  L.  tapped  longer  strokes 
for  the  unpleasant  than  for  the  indifferent,  while  B.  and  T. 
tapped  longer  strokes  for  the  pleasant  than  for  the  neutral. 
A  comparison  of  these  results  with  those  given  previously  shows 
very  little  constancy. 

THE  PSYCHO-MOTOR  EFFECTS  OF  N 

The  next  seven  experiments  were  constructed  to  ascertain  the 
effect  of  accented  N;  the  unaccented  syllable,  "be"  (short  e) 
was  chosen  because  it  seemed  to  be  about  as  explosive  as  N, 
and  thus  would  be  a  good  balance  for  it.  The  experiments 
were:  be-ne  (e  short),  be-ne  (e  long),  be-ni  (i  short),  be-ni 
(  i  long),  be-na  (a  long),  be-nu  (u  short)  and  be-noo  (oo  long). 
N  is  also  a  much  used  sound  in  the  language.  Seeing  that  these 
experiments  furnished  a  better  body  of  material  than  any  pairs 
or  triads  which  had  preceded,  it  was  decided  to  correlate  accord- 
ing to  the  mean,  the  mean  variation,  and  the  range.  In- 
trospectionally,  N  appeared  to  arouse  an  attitude  of  negation. 
This  group  of  experiments  also  appeared  as  the  conjugation 
of  a  verb,  and  took  on  at  once  with  most  of  the  subjects  a  dis- 
tinctly "oriental"  character.  The  N  dominated  consciousness, 
in  spite  of  the  changing  final  vowel.  The  experiments  were  all 
given  on  the  same  day ;  which  may  also  account  for  the  following 
constancy  in  numerical  results. 

Rank  list.     Average  of  the  tappings   for  each   subject. 


Subject 
A  

Be-ne 
..    ..     b 

-ne 
b 

-m 
c 

-ni 
c 

-na 
b 

-nu 
b 

-noo 
b 

Variation 

B  

h 

h 

h 

F. 

e 

e 

e 

d 

g 

d 

L  

.     ..     d 

d 

d 

e 

d 

H 

N,    . 

h 

h 

h 

h 

T  

f 

f 

f 

f 

f 

f 

f 

W.   . 

c 

c 

b 

b 

c 

Z. 

a 

a 

a 

a 

a 

a 

a 

n 

I4  ROBERT  CHENAULT  GIVLER 

If,  however,  we  take  the  averages  of  the  tappings  for  the 
pleasant,  unpleasant  and  neutral  experiments,  no  such  harmony 

is  manifest.    As  follows: 

PUN 

Average  of  arranged  in 

Subject                                       PUN  order  of  magnitude 

A 52-9           53-3            514  U  P  N 

B 78.0           76.7           80.4  N  P  U 

F.    62.9           62.4           65.0  N  P  U 

L 62.3           62.5           65.0  PUN 

N 78-5           80.8            —  UP  — 

T 68.0           68.1           65.2  UPN 

W 53-8 

Z 46.0           49.1  UP  — 

But  even  if  no  judgments  of  unpleasantness  or  neutrality  were 
made  by  some  of  the  subjects,  yet  the  above  table  shows  that 
when  such  judgments  were  made,  it  was  not  at  a  time  when  the 
tappings  were  the  longest;  one  is  again  at  this  place  referred 
to  Fere's  "sensation  et  mouvement",  op.  cit.  Where  comparison 
is  possible  in  these  above  citations,  rarely  did  the  pleasant  feeling 
tone  go  with  the  longest  tapped  strokes.  The  balance  hangs 
almost  evenly  between  neutrality  and  unpleasantness  in  this 
respect. 

The  rank  lists  of  the  mean  variations,  hereafter  denominated 
by  M.V.  and  of  the  ranges,  that  is,  the  millimetric  distance 
between  the  longest  and  the  shortest  tapped  strokes,  denominated 
later  by  Rnj,  show  no  positive  correlation.  In  this  instance, 
also,  the  averages  of  the  M.V.  and  of  the  Rnj.  for  the  P.,  U., 
and  N,  experiments  is  hardly  significant. 

The  graphs  for  these  experiments  showed  that  be-ne  and  be-ne 
are  similar  in  their  capacity  to  arouse  equal  amounts  of  motor 
discharge.  The  average  difference  is  but  2mm.  All  things  con- 
sidered, the  increase  of  motor  output  was  fairly  steady  from 
start  to  finish.  Bern  and  Be-ni  showed  a  less  increase  from 
start  to  finish  and  in  the  third  and  fourth  group  of  five  iambics 
stood  somewhat  apart.  There  was  a  general  rise  in  be-nu,  but 
be-noo  fell  almost  precipitously  at  the  close.  Be-na  tended  to 
duplicate  be-ne  and  be-ne.  These  differences  can  hardly  be 
correlated  with  those  of  feeling  tone,  for  the  be-ne  was  found 
to  be  pleasant  by  3  persons,  Unp,  by  4,  and  indifferent  by  one; 
be-ne,  which  followed  it  quite  faithfully  in  the  graph,  was 


ELEMENTS  OF  SPEECH  IN  RELATION  TO  POETRY         15 

chosen  pleasant  by  7,  and  Unp.  by  one.  Likewise,  be-nu  was 
found  pleasant  by  two  persons,  Unp.  by  three,  and  yet  this  graph 
does  not  ascend  at  all  like  the  other  one  or  with  so  great  an  up- 
ward slant;  moreover,  be-noo,  which  was  chosen  pleasant  by 
all  the  subjects  (including  the  7  who  chose  be-ne  as  pleasant), 
did  not  produce  the  same  kind  of  a  graph  in  appearance  as  did 
be-ni.  Furthermore,  there  was  no  report  from  the  subjects  that 
they  felt  the  finger  strokes  getting  longer  or  shorter  in  any 
such  way  as  these  graphs  indicate  they  must  have  done.  And 
every  one  of  the  subjects  contributed  to  the  increases  and  de- 
creases. One  can  but  conjecture  then,  that  some  of  the  neural 
currents  find  their  way  out  of  the  central  system  along  that 
motor  channel  which  is  already  in  use,  without  making  their 
functional  nature  known  to  the  introspective  consciousness.  It 
was  unpleasant,  also,  for  nearly  all  of  the  subjects  to  be  aware 
of  their  lip  and  tongue  movements;  and  while  some  of  them 
actually  did  raise  the  pitch  of  their  voices  at  the  finish  of  be-na 
and  be-ni,  yet  they  had  no  notion  of  it,  much  less  of  the  fact 
that  they  were  tapping  in  co-ordination  with  this  general  in- 
crease of  effort.  The  only  introspection  they  gave  on  this  matter 
was  "a  feeling  of  difficulty"  (e.g.  with  be-ni)  and  a  "feeling  of 
activity"  etc.  (e.g.  with  be-ni).  That  both  of  these  should 
produce  the  same  general  increase  of  tapping  is  interesting. 

THE  EFFECT  OF  LONG  OO 

The  next  experiments  were  of  the  same  general  character. 
Ro  (long  o)  was  chosen  as  the  unaccented  foot,  and  the  long 
accented  vowel  was  oo  (long).  Both  being  long,  open  vowels, 
a  good  balance  was  expected.  Furthermore,  the  long  oo  vanishes 
quite  readily  into  a  long  o,  preceded  by  R.  Unlike  the  former 
group,  which  was  devised  to  study  the  effect  of  accented  N,  this 
group  intended  to  bring  into  prominence  the  mouth  resonances, 
rather  than  the  articulation  pressure  of  the  consonant  N.  The 
following  consonants,  in  the  following  order,  were  prefixed  to 
the  accented  long  oo:  B,  M,  V,  TH  (sonant),  D,  Z,  SH, 
J,  L,  and  G.  As  usual,  the  iambic  decasyllabic  line  was  employed, 
and  repeated  five  times. 


16  ROBERT  CHENAULT  GIVLER 

From  the  introspection  given  for  these  experiments  it  was 
found  that  the  long  vowels  employed  in  them  dominated  the 
combinations  and  had  a  non-personal  reference.  Frequently  the 
effect  became  soporific,  and  again,  when  the  consciousness  of 
facial  expression  involved  while  reciting  them  became  observed, 
the  subjects  were  inclined  to  call  the  emotion  thus  induced  one 
of  "supplication"  or  "complaint."  None  of  the  consonants  at- 
tached to  this  vowel  ever  became  at  all  "hard,"  or  difficult  to  say. 

The  rank  list  for  the  mean  of   these  experiments. 

Experiment  Ro-boo  -moo  -voo    -thoo  -doo    -zoo  -shoo     -joo     -loo     -goo 
Subject 


A  

d 

e 

e 

e 

f 

d 

e 

d 

e 

f 

B  

e 

d 

g 

g 

d 

f 

f 

f 

f 

e 

C  

i 

i 

i 

i 

i 

i 

i 

i 

i 

i* 

F  

f 

f 

c 

d 

e 

e 

d 

e 

d 

d 

L  

b 

c 

b 

b 

b 

b 

c 

b 

b 

b 

N  

g 

h 

f 

f 

h 

g 

g 

g 

h 

g 

T  

c 

b 

d 

c 

c 

c 

b 

c 

c 

c 

W  

h 

g 

h 

h 

g 

h 

h 

h 

g 

h 

Y  

a 

a 

a 

a 

a 

a 

a 

a 

a 

at 

Comparing  these  ranks  with  those  of  the  be-ni  type  of  experi- 
ment and  with  only  those  subjects  who  took  part  in  both,  we 
find  them  showing  the  following  divergences  from  a  steady 
position : 

Be-ni,   etc.,    A.  2,  B.  3,  F.  2,  L.  2,  N.  3,  T.  o,  W.  2. 

Ro-boo,   etc.,    . . .  A.  5,  B.  6,  F.  8,  L.  2,  N.  5,  T.  3,  W.  3. 

The  first  group  represented  here  contained  7  experiments,  the 
second,  10.  One  can  expect  a  wider  latitude  of  variation  in  a 
greater  quantity  of  material.  F.,  alone  seems  to  have  increased 
the  ratio  of  divergence  more  than  would  be  expected.  It  was 
noticed,  also,  that  the  organic  stirs  which  some  of  these  experi- 
ments, like,  for  example,  Ro-thoo  set  up,  was  not  shaken  off 
by  him  (F.)  until  several  of  the  subsequent  experiments  had 
been  performed.  Besides,  -moo  and  -voo  had  disturbed  his 
original  position  in  the  ranks,  which  was  "f."  He  never  after- 
wards regained  it  in  this  set  of  experiments. 

The  introspection  for  the  above  experiments  contained  many 
expressions  of  "feelings  of  activity,"  "struggles,"  "quiet  states," 

*, Subject  C.  now  begins  to  tap  the  longest  strokes  of  any  and  f  Subject  Y. 
remains  in  the  lowest  position.  It  is  the  rank  of  the  other  seven  subjects, 
who  varied  the  length  of  their  tappings  most  and  whose  tappings  are  nearer 
alike  in  length  which  needs  to  be  considered  especially. 


ELEMENTS  OF  SPEECH  IN  RELATION  TO  POETRY         17 

and  so  on.  Let  us  compare  these  with  the  amounts  of  motor 
discharge  in  the  tappings : 

A.  felt  "-moo"  to  be  easy,  and  "thoo"  to  be  a  struggle;  and 
the  M.  for  -moo  is  greater  than  for  -thoo;  again,  in  -loo,  he 
felt  activity,  but  the  M.  for  -loo  is  not  as  high  as  it  is  for  -shoo, 
-joo,  and  -goo,  in  each  of  which  there  were  unpleasant  mouth 
sensations.  B.  felt  -joo  to  be  the  most  energetic,  and  his  M.  for 
this  is  the  highest  of  the  series,  94.8;  -thoo,  which  brought 
visual  imagery  of  a  dense  crowd,  was  accompanied  by  a  M.  of 
94.0;  -zoo,  with  "openness,"  has  an  M.  of  91.0;  but  while  with 
th,  "crowdedness"  is  correlated  with  94.8  mm.,  "sultriness"  in 
the  imagery  of  -doo  is  correlated  with  only  77.6  mm.  in  the  mean 
of  the  tappings. 

C.  shows  some  nice  correlation  between  energetic  and  passive 
states  in  connection  with  -boo,  -moo  and  -voo ;  but  -loo  is  higher 
in  the  M.  than  -joo.  F.  gave  the  lowest  of  his  M.'s  to  -voo  and 
-thoo,  which  he  found  the  most  difficult  to  say,  while  in  the  sounds 
which  brought  a  feeling  of  activity,  he  taps  the  longest  strokes. 
L.  does  not  seem  to  offer  correlation  either  way.  N.  during 
states  in  which  activity  is  felt,  taps  longer  strokes  than  when 
some  restraint  is  manifest;  c.f.  -voo,  -doo,  -zoo  and  -loo  as 
compared  with  -boo,  -thoo,  and  -joo.  T.  and  Y.  do  not  seem 
to  furnish  any  definite  correlations.  W.  taps  variously  for  the 
energetic  sounds,  yet  gives  a  M.  of  99.8  to  -zoo,  which  did  not 
seem  to  appear  energetic  to  him. 

Thus  the  three  factors  of  "free  activity,"  "restraint"  and  "quiet 
ease"  do  not  correlate  with  the  amounts  of  motor  discharge  in 
any  way  as  one  might  expect.  Three  classes  of  subjects  are  evi- 
denced in  the  above  records ; — those  who  tend  towards  relaxation 
in  the  finger  when  they  feel  it  in  the  imagery  or  in  the  utter- 
ance of  the  sounds,  those  who  do  the  opposite,  and  those  who  vary 
throughout  the  experiments.  But  it  is  perhaps  too  early  in  the 
work  to  make  any  general  statements. 

Correlation  of  feeling-tone  with  motor  discharge 

Experiments  :  ro-boo ro-goo,  as  before 

Subjects  PI.          Unpl.      Neutral 

A 83.6  84.2  84.9  N  U  P 

B 89.3  77-6  85.7  P  N  U 

C 103.1          108.8         109.2  N  U  P 


L    

.  .1    61.4 

64.2 

N    

01.6 

90.4 

T               

85.5 

76.1- 

06.0 

Y  

46.6 

46.0 

18  ROBERT  CHENAULT  CILLER 

—  P  U  — 
63.0  U  N  P 

—  P  U  — 

—  P  U  — 
—           88.8                     P  N  — 

—  P  U  — 

This  seems  to  give  a  decidedly  different  sort  of  result  from 
that  of  the  Be-ni  type  of  experiment  revealed  with  respect  to  the 
length  of  the  tappings  during  the  pleasant  experiments;  but 
if  the  single  experiments  are  taken  into  consideration,  it  will 
be  found  that  while  subjects  A.  L.  T.  and  W.  do  tap  the  longest 
strokes  for  the  most  pleasant  of  the  pleasant  experiments,  yet 
the  other  subjects  do  not  do  so;  B.  C.  F.  and  N.  show  no  prefer- 
ences, while  Y.  taps  the  shortest.  But  this  division  of  the  sub- 
jects into  classes  does  not  run  parallel  with  the  above  division  into 
classes  on  the  basis  of  motor  output  and  feelings  of  restraint, 
activity  or  quiet  ease.  But  until  we  come  to  a  set  of  experiments 
in  which  each  one  of  the  subjects  is  represented  in  all  three  feel- 
ing tones,  it  is  hardly  fair  to  pit  one  set  of  results  over  against 
another  to  the  detriment  of  either.  It  may  well  be  that  the 
continuance  of  pleasant  states  or  of  other  kinds  has  its  own  special 
effect  upon  the  motor  resources. 

Considering  the  three  rank  lists,  M.,  M.V.  and  Rnj.  together, 
find  that  the  number  of  aberrations  from  a  steady  position  is  very 
great,  and  only  the  following  remarks  are  appropriate : 

i.  Subjects  C.,  F.  and  N.,  approach  and  sometimes  maintain 
some  degree  of  regularity  in  the  Mean  Variation  and  in  the 
Range,  yet  only  one  of  these,  C.,  remained  steady  in  the  Mean 
rank  list.  Here,  in  the  M.V.,  this  subject  has  four  f-positions 
and  in  the  Rnj.,  three  g-positions,  with  a  general  tendency  to 
maintain  them.  Subject  F.  who  had  eight  displacements  in  the 
Mean  rank  list,  steadies  himself  with  three  h-positions  in  the 
M.V.  rank  list,  and  with  three  g-positions  in  the  Rnj.  rank  list, 
but  only  in  the  latter  does  he  tend  toward  making  that  letter  his 
moorings.  Subject  N.,  with  five  displacements  in  the  Mean 
rank  list,  shows  here  steadiness  in  the  M.V.  list,  tending  toward 
an  a-position,  and  in  the  Rnj.  list  his  tendency  is  toward  maintain- 
ing the  same  position  also.  But  Y,  who  was  absolutely  steady  in 


ELEMENTS  OF  SPEECH  IN  RELATION  TO  POETRY         19 

the  Mean  rank  list,  here  shows  only  three  h-positions  in  the  M.V. 
and  but  three  c-positions  in  the  Rnj.  rank  list. 

2.  Most  of  the  other  subjects,  who  tended  toward  some  steadi- 
ness in  the  Mean  rank  list,  here  are  scattered  up  and  down  the 
scales  in  complete  disorder. 

Correlations  were  shown  before  between  the  feeling  tone  and 
the  averages  of  all  the  tappings  for  the  pleasant,  unpleasant  and 
neutral  combinations.  There  follows  a  similar  table,  showing 
the  correlation  between  the  feeling  tone  and  averages  of  all 
the  mean  variations  and  ranges  of  all  the  tappings  during  the 
various  feeling  tone  states. 

(Repeating  the  former  correlations  with  the  mean.) 

Experiments  :  ro-boo ro-goo. 

Subjects  Pleas.      Unpl.      Neut.         (Mean)        N  U  P 

A.  M.V 3.1  3.4  2.4  U  P  N 

Rnj 18  19  12  U  P  N 

U 

B.  M.V 3.7  3.2  4.1 

Rnj 20  13  19 

(i 

N  U  P 

C.  M.V 4.5  3.7  2.9  PUN 

Rnj 19  24  16  U  P  N 

« 

P  U* 
F.  M.V 3-5  3-0  P  U 

Rnj 20  18  PU 

U 

U  N  P 
L.  M.V 3-5  34  4-9  N  P  U 

Rnj 18  17  23  NPU 

<( 

P  U 
N.  M.V 2.2  2.9  UP 

Rnj 12  14  —  UP 

it 

P  U* 
T.  M.V 3-8  3-4  P  U 

iRnj 20  19  —  P  U 

a 

P  N 

W.  M.V , 3-0          2.7  P  N 

Rnj 16  —         17  N  P 

1C 

P  U 

Y.  M.V 3.1  3.0  P  U 

Rnj 17  22  UP 

*  Where  only  two  kinds  of  affective  judgments  are  made,  of  course  the 
possibility  of  correlation  is  better,  but  even  chance  would  give  as  good 
correlations  as  N.,  W.,  and  Y.  show. 


20  ROBERT  CHENAULT  GIVLER 

The  graphs  for  these  experiments  showed  an  entirely  new  char- 
acter in  the  visible  record  of  the  average  of  the  tappings  for  all 
the  subjects.  With  few  exceptions,  the  rise  is  only  initial,  but 
not  a  great  deal  of  importance  is  to  be  given  to  the  first  group  of 
five  iambics  in  any  of  the  experiments,  because  no  preliminary 
tapping  was  done  by  any  of  the  subjects ;  they  all  began  to  tap  and 
recite  at  the  same  time.  The  averages  of  the  Be-ni  type  of  ex- 
periment were  all  below  70  mm.;  these  are  all  above  77  mm.; 
evidently  all  the  subjects  got  more  familiar  with  the  work.  In- 
deed, all  of  them  seem  to  have  by  this  time  passed  the  period 
of  the  "Anregung,"  as  can  be  easily  demonstrated  from  the 
tables  which  are  to  follow.  It  remains  to  be  seen  whether  the 
subjects  respond  to  the  material  of  the  experiment  in  such  a  way 
as  to  furnish  correlations  between  feeling-tone  and  motor  dis- 
charge in  point  of  Mean,  Mean  variation  and  Range  that  will  be 
of  any  service  in  determining  the  psycho-motor  effect  of  the 
speech  elements  in  poetry.  Referring  to  the  graphs  again,  it 
appears  that  the  vowel  OO  swallows  the  consonants  which  pre- 
cede it,  and  to  produce  in  the  drawings  the  visible  effect  of  OO 
rather  than  of  B,  M,  TH  and  so  forth.  It  remains  to  be  seen 
whether  the  other  vowels  to  be  experimented  upon  perform  this 
same  usurpative  function  or  not.  It  will  be  remembered  that 
OO  dominated  also  the  introspective  consciousness  in  these 
experiments. 

THE  EFFECT  OF  LONG  E 

The  next  ten  experiments  were  devised  to  exhibit  the  effect  of 
long  E.  The  unaccented  syllable  was  "la"  (given  as  the  Italian 
A,  but  it  immediately  became  the  neutral  vowel). 

In  general,  the  effect  of  "E"  was  to  produce  feelings  of  tension, 
and  as  a  long  vowel,  it  was  thought  to  take  decidedly  less  time 
than  either  O  or  OO.  Some  of  the  subjects  tried  to  "put  force 
into  it,"  but  did  not  succeed ;  it  appeared  to  cause  restraint,  rather 
than  the  "expected  sense  of  outward  control."  All  of  the  subjects 
called  its  pitch  very  high  and  not  at  all  like  the  effect  of  most 
words  containing  long  E's.  It  also  appeared  to  be  more  modified 


ELEMENTS  OF  SPEECH  IN  RELATION  TO  POETRY         21 

by  the  consonants  preceding  it  than  were  the  vowels  in  the  other 
previously  given  experiments. 

From  constructing  the  rank  lists  for  the  mean  of  the  tappings 
for  these  experiments,  one  sees  greater  variation  from  a  steady 
position  than  with  either  of  the  two  previously  given  groups 
of  experiments.  As  follows : 

Be-ni    A.  2    6.3    C.  3    F.  2    L.  2    N.  3    T.  o    W.  2    Y.  o 

Ro-boo     5          6         8         8         2          5          3  3         o 

-foe   10        in         89379  30 

Should  we  ask  whether  the  change  in  the  rank  for  each  subject 
denotes  a  change  in  the  feeling  tone,  the  answer  is  doubtfully 
given  either  way.  Y.  found  all  these  experiments  pleasant,  and 
keeps  the  same  rank,  but  this  subject's  tappings  are  way  lower 
in  length  than  any  of  the  other  subjects'.  W.,  who  finds  the 
last  nine  of  these  experiments  pleasant  and  who  varies  very  little 
in  his  position  in  the  ranks,  may  be  said  to  be  fairly  constant, 
but  subject  B.,  who  also  found  the  last  nine  experiments  pleasant, 
varies  his  position  in  the  ranks  more  than  any  of  the  other 
subjects  (n  points).  L.,  who  is  quite  steady,  varies  his  posi- 
tion even  when  he  finds  consecutive  experiments  are  equally  pleas- 
ant or  otherwise;  while  N.,  who  finds  the  first  eight  experiments 
pleasant  varies  most  during  the  first  part  of  the  rank  list. 

Let  us  examine  once  more  the  averages  of  the  tappings  with 
reference  to  feelings  of  activity,  hindrance  and  the  like.  A.  felt 
a  strain  while  reciting  -fe,  and  his  average  is  low;  -ke  and  -le, 
which  were  felt  to  be  active,  show  high  averages.  C.  found  -ne 
more  energetic  than  -le,  but  tapped  shorter  strokes  for  it ;  -ge  he 
found  to  be  "powerful,"  and  his  average  tapping  is  the  highest 
for  this  sound.  But  -che  is  also  quite  active,  and  yet  the  average 
of  the  tappings  is  low.  He  thought  he  was  tapping  very  long 
strokes  for  -the,  but  he  was  mistaken.  F.  began  to  feel  strain 
sensations  with  the  recitation  of  -ne,  and  from  this  point  on  he 
taps  longer  strokes;  he  called  -ke  less  free  than  -le,  and  taps 
longer  strokes  for  the  latter  sound.  When  L.  found  -ke  a  "hard" 
sound,  his  tappings  were  lower.  Usually,  the  more  "harmonious 
states  of  mind"  brought  the  lowest  averages  for  N.'s  tappings. 
But  when  W.  felt  the  freest,  his  tappings  were  the  longest.  Sub- 


22  ROBERT  CHENAULT  GIVLER 

jects  A.,  F.,  N.  and  possibly  W.  seem  to  be  keeping  quite  constant ; 
they  tap  the  longest  strokes  in  the  freely  active  states,  and 
vice  versa. 

Correlation  between  feeling-tone  and  motor-discharge,  with  respect  to  the 
mean  of  all  the  experiments,  grouped  under  the  three  degrees  of  affect,  P.,  U., 
and  N. 

Experiments :  -be -ge. 

Subjects                                              PI.  U               N 

A.., 85.0  81.3           85.0  PNU 

B 83.0  —           87.4  N  P 

C 93-1  85.6           86.6  UPN 

F 81.5  77.0  P  U 

L 68.3  74-8           65.5  UPN 

N 88.5  90.2  N  P 

T 79.3  76.2           78.0  PNU 

W 94-8  91.8            —  P  U 

Y    50.8  —            — 

Comparing  this  with  the  correlations  for  the  -boo  experiments, 
we  find  F.,  L.,  T.  and  W.  somewhat  similar  in  their  preferences, 
but  the  other  subjects  vary  exceedingly.  L.,  indeed  is  the  only 
one  giving  three  judgments  who  duplicates  himself. 

Of  all  the  subjects,  Y.  appears  the  most  constant  all  the  way 
through.  N.'s  ranges  rank  fairly  steadily,  but  the  Mean  is  not 
constant.  W.  and  L.  represent  the  best  averages,  after  Y.  C., 
who  varies  much  in  the  F.T.  as  the  experiments  proceed,  also 
varies  much  in  these  rank  lists,  but  other  subjects  do  not  correlate 
in  the  same  way. 

Correlation  between  feeling  tone  and  the  averages  of  all  the  M.V.'s  and 
Rnj.'s  for  the  various  experiments  (together  with  the  previously  given  data 
for  the  Mean). 

Experiments  :   -be -ge. 

Subjects  Pleas.      Unpl.      Neut.          (Mean)        PNU 

A.  M.V 4.3  5.4  2.6  UPN 

Rnj 23  21  13  PUN 

N  P 

B.  M.V 4.1  3.0  P  N 

Rnj 48  —         ii  PN 

UPN 

C.  M.V 3.9  2.7  2.3  PUN 

Rnj 21  19  20  PNU 

P  U 

F.  M.V 3.9  3.2          —  P  U 

Rnj 16  18  UP 

UPN 

L.  M.V 3.4  3.7  4.8  N  U  P 

Rnj 15  16  18  N  U  P 

N  P 


ELEMENTS  OF  SPEECH  IN  RELATION  TO  POETRY      23 
N.  M.V 2.3        2.1 

Rnj 21  ii  — 

« 

T.  M.V 4-2  34  4-6 

Rnj 21  14  16 

« 

W.M.V 3-6  3-5          — 

Rnj 23  21 

Y.  M.V 2.8          

Rnj 14 

Here  W.  alone  remains  constant.  All  the  rest  vary  almost 
as  much  as  is  possible  with  three  permutable  terms. 

Something  must  be  said  now  in  regard  to  the  last  two  sets 
of  experiments  in  point  of  constancy  in  tapping  during  all  the 
pleasant,  unpleasant  and  neutral  states.  We  observe  that  the 
final  average  of  the  mean,  mean  variation  and  the  range  do  not 
adequately  represent  in  most  of  the  cases  the  general  results.  If 
one  studies  the  variations  from  these  averages,  he  will  see  that 
especially  in  the  "pleasant"  experiments,  there  is  almost  no 
confidence  to  be  put  in  these  figures  as  representative.  It  is  not 
so  much  so  in  the  case  of  the  "unpleasant"  experiments.  Space 
does  not  permit  a  full  review  of  this  interesting  point,  but  in 
general,  the  pleasant  states  have  more  varied  ways  of  represent- 
ing themselves  in  the  tapping  than  do  the  others.  The  subjects 
frequently  show  that  there  is  more  variation  from  the  mean  of 
the  "pleasant"  tappings  when  there  is  no  interruption  in  the 
affective  tone  as  the  list  precedes,  than  when  some  other  condi- 
tion is  manifest.  This  is  very  curious.  And  the  objections  that 
might  be  brought  against  any  such  method  of  experiment  seem 
now  to  have  plenty  of  reasons  for  their  existence.  Some  might 
say  that  there  should  have  been  more  careful  judgments  on  the 
degrees  of  pleasure,  to  obtain  a  more  accurate  correlation,  but  it 
was  deemed  a  very  arbitrary  matter  to  oblige  the  subjects  to  say 
""pleasure  i,  2,  or  3"  when  they  did  not  feel  such  a  difference  to 
be  manifest. 

Our  next  interest  is  in  the  graphs  for  these  long  E  experiments. 
Ke,  -ne,  -ve,  -me,  and  -le  all  start  stronger  than  do  the  others, 
and  also  move  straight  across  the  page;  while  the  others  ascend 
fairly  well  together,  but  end  in  different  degrees  of  strength. 


24  ROBERT  CHENAULT  GIVLER 

The  "scatter"  of  the  first  group  of  these  graphs  is  greater  in 
the  second  and  third  groups  of  iambics, — that  of  the  others  (the 
-be,  -ge,  -the,  -che,  and  -fe)  is  prominent  only  in  the  last  two 
groups.  One  might  say  that  L,  M,  N,  and  V,  being  pleasant,  had 
here  shown  positive  correlation  between  pleasantness  and  motor 
discharge,  since  they  are  all  lower  than  those  of  the  most  unpleas- 
ant experiments,  those  employing  K,  G,  B,  and  TH,  but  -ke  is  in 
the  group  which  shows  the  less  motor  discharge.  Also  -che, 
which  everybody  found  pleasant,  is  next  to  the  very  topmost 
graph  of  the  lot,  which  means  that  its  average  is  to  be  placed  with 
the  other  pleasant  experiments.  Ranking  the  experiments  in  a 
descending  order  of  pleasantness,  below  which  are  the  final  aver- 
ages of  all  the  tappings,  it  can  be  shown,  that  with  the  exception 

Exp -che      -le       -ne       -me      -ve       -fe      -the       -ke      -ge       -be 

Av 80.2      85.8      82.8     83.2      82.0      78.6      81.2      86.6*    78.4      78.4 

of  -ke*,  the  correlation  runs  positive  with  the  pleasantness.  As 
for  -ke,  it  was  chosen  as  indifferent  by  two  of  the  subjects.  But 
this  apparent  correlation  may  be  due  to  the  tapping  of  but  one 
subject,  W.,  who  gave  the  longer  tappings  to  the  pleasant  com- 
binations. 

The  next  experiments  to  be  tried  were  devised  with  a  view  of 
discovering  the  effect  of  the  "aw"  sound.  They  were  five  in 
number.  The  unaccented  syllable  was  De,  (long  E).  Aw  was 
preceded  by  these  consonants :  f ,  th,  t,  n,  and  g. 

Introspectively  it  proved  to  appear  pitched  very  low,  to  have 
a  tendency  to  become  nasal ;  not  very  musical,  but  arousing  more 
organic  stir  than  any  sound  previously  used. 

The  rank  list  for  the  Mean  of  these  experiments  is,  with  symbols  P,  U,  N 
indicating  the  feeling  tone,  as  follows : 

Experiment:  -faw  -thaw  -taw  -naw  -gaw 

Subject 

A.    d-P  c-U  c-P  c-P  c-P 

B f-P  d-U  f-P  f-P  h-P 


F.   , 

C,N 

&  «" 
e-P 

e-P 

d-U 

e-N 

L  

b-p 

b-U 

b-P 

b-U 

b-N 

N  

h-p 

h-P 

R-P 

g-P 

f-P 

T.   , 

e-P 

f-P 

d-P 

e-N 

d-N 

W  

i-N 

i-U 

h-P 

i-P 

ff-N 

Y.  . 

a-N 

a-N 

a-N 

a-N 

a-N 

ELEMENTS  OF  SPEECH  IN  RELATION  TO  POETRY         25 

Notice  here  that  subject  Y.  would  hold  position  -a-  in  the 
ranks,  regardless  of  feeling  tone;  and  that  L.,  whose  position  in 
the  ranks  is  -b-  in  these  experiments,  shows  here  lower  tapping 
averages  than  he  has  for  some  time.  It  is  hardly  possible  to 
make  any  statement  about  these  ranks,  except  to  say  that  the 
subjects  are  all  more  anchored  to  one  position  than  in  the  case 
of  either  the  Ro-boo  or  the  La-be  experiments. 

In  regard  to  feelings  of  effort  and  activity,  C.  described  -naw 
as  requiring  effort,  but  the  average  for  this  experiment  is  almost 
the  lowest  of  the  series.  -Taw,  which  gave  a  feeling  of  activity, 
is  correspondingly  high,  but  -thaw,  which  affected  him  the  same 
way,  fails  to  show  in  these  averages ;  -f aw,  also  requiring  effort, 
is  parallel  in  effect  to  -naw.  The  other  subjects  do  not  furnish 
enough  examples  to  make  correlation  exact. 

Grouping  the  above  results  according  to  feeling  tone,  and 
taking  their  averages,  we  obtain : 

Subjects                                       P.  U.  N 

A 64.9  66.7  UP 

B 80.9  73.6  P  U 

C 88.6  85.3  97-8                               N  P  U 

F 76.4         71-3         70-5  PUN 

L 57-8         55-0          58.0  N  P  U 

N „ 86.6 

T 75-0          73  5  P  N  — 

W 90.6         97.1          87.2  U  P  N 

Y 28.9  

which,  as  results  along  this  line,  are  not  paralllel  with  any  that 
have  been  obtained  before. 

The  rank  lists  for  the  M.V.  and  the  Rnj.  show  that  of  all  the 
subjects,  only  W.  and  Y.  keep  some  sort  of  anchorage  in  them; 
the  rest  vary  indiscriminately. 

Grouping  (and  averaging)  all  the  Means,  Mean-variations  and 
Ranges  according  to  feeling  tone,  we  obtain  the  following: 

(Order  of  greatest  to  least) 
Subject 

A.  M.  UP 

M.V UP 

Rnj P  U 


.26  ROBERT  CHENAULT  GIVLER 

B.  (as  a'bove)  P  U 

P  U 
P  U 

C.  .  N  P  U 

P  N  U 
P  N  U 

F.  .  PUN 

N  U  P 
N  P  U 


L N  P  U 

PUN 
N  P  U 

N all  P 

T.  .  P  N 

P  N 
P  N 

W.  .  U  P  N 

PUN 
U  P  N 

Y all  N 

From  the  graphs  for  these  experiments  it  appears  that  -gaw 
ascends  steadily;  -ge,  in  the  preceding  series,  on  the  other  hand, 
maintained  a  horizontal  position.  -Naw  and  -ne  are  likewise 
opposite  in  tendency,  indicating  some  comparison  in  regard  to 
these  consonants.  But  -faw  and  -fe  show  the  very  opposite  traits, 
as  well  as  do  -thaw  and  -the.  One  is  tempted  to  correlate  with 
the  articulation-character  of  these  consonants,  but  the  amount 
of  experimentation  is  as  yet  too  meager.  However,  one  thing 
more  may  be  noticed,  and  that  is  that  all  of  these  -aw  experi- 
ments but  one,  namely  -naw,  show  in  their  final  averages  that 
the  vowel  "aw"  tends  to  swallow  up  the  consonants,  in  a  way 
that  the  vowel  "e"  never  did.  Time  did  not  permit  any  further 
experimentation  with  this  vowel  sound;  furthermore,  it  is  not  a 
very  important  one  in  the  tables  of  sound  frequencies  for  English 
poetry.  For  equal  bulks  of  material,  nevertheless,  and  for  those 
consonants  which  were  used  alike  before  accented  -aw  and  -e,  it  is 
not  idle  to  point  to  these  results  as  showing  something  quite 
significant  in  the  psycho-motor  effect  of  the  speech  elements  of 
poetry.  Any  one  can  see  that  the  whole  matter  is  one  of  amazing 


ELEMENTS  OF  SPEECH  IN  RELATION  TO  POETRY         27 

complexity;  later  results  may  induce  some  sort  of  generalization 
of  a  specific  character,  but  whether  pro  or  con  the  matter  of 
vocal  valences,  cannot  be  foretold  at  this  time. 

EXPERIMENTS  UPON  THE  FOUR  MOST  USED  LONG 
VOWELS  IN  ENGLISH  POETRY:  A,  E,  I,  O 

We  next  undertook  an  extended  study  of  the  psycho-motor 
effect  of  the  long  vowels  A,  O,  I  and  E.  Ten  subjects  took  part 
in  the  experiments. 

The  experimental  material  was  made  on  this  plan:  The  un- 
accented syllable  was  "la"  (neutral  vowel),  and  the  various 
consonants  were  prefixed  to  each  of  the  above  vowels  to  make 
such  combinations  as  "La-BA,"  "La-DA,"  "La-CHE,"  etc. 
there  were  twenty-four  experiments  on  each  vowel,  which  at 
the  same  time  were  experiments  on  each  of  the  consonants  em- 
ployed; thus  we  had  four  experiments  in  which  the  consonant  B 
was  used,  and  so  on  for  all  the  series.  The  experiments  were  all 
given  to  each  of  the  subjects  in  the  same  order,  at  the  rate  of 
about  eight  or  ten  an  hour. 

The  last  two  experiments  in  each  series  of  twenty-four  are 
slightly  different  from  the  rest ;  in  the  tables  presented  later  they 
are  called  A,  A2,  O,  O2,  etc.  The  twenty-third  experiment  in 
each  series,  A,  O,  I,  E,  consisted  simply  in  reciting  the  open 
vowel  five  times  in  a  group  and  for  five  groups,  filling  in  the 
unaccented  syllable  subjectively.  Experiment  number  twenty- 
four  in  each  group  is  a  line  of  verse  in  which  all  the  accented 
vowels  are  the  same,  thus :  A2  is  the  line,  "The  gray  and  rainy 
April  makes  the  May."  O2  is  "The  homeless  ocean  moaning  o'er 
the  shoal" ;  12  is,  "The  dying  fire  lights  the  silent  sky,"  and  E2 
is  "And  dreaming  seem  to  hear  the  weary  sea."  No  such  lines 
exist  in  poetry,  but  they  served  the  purpose  in  hand. 

It  was  found  by  introspection  that  the  characteristic  thing 
about  the  long  A  was  its  "flat  and  uninteresting"  quality.  It  was 
by  no  means  energetic,  and  when  liked,  it  was  termed  "soft  and 
quiet" ;  vocally  it  was  felt  to  be  directed  downwards  rather  than 
upwards.  As  far  as  the  vowel-vanish  is  concerned,  it  was  so 
little  noticed  by  the  subjects  that  we  need  not  mention  it;  of  course 


2&  ROBERT  CHENAULT  GIVLER 

the  repetition  of  the  same  iambic  foot  brought  it  so  closely  into 
contact  with  L  in  the  unaccented  syllable,  as  almost  to  nullify  the 
effect  of  the  vanish. 

One  rather  remarkable  thing  was  noticed  in  the  numerical  re- 
sults for  the  A2  experiments:  The  mean  of  the  tappings  for 
every  subject  in  this  experiment  drops  below  what  it  was  for 
the  previous  experiment,  A;  with  most  of  the  subjects,  also,  it 
is  lower  than  their  average  for  all  the  other  A  experiments.  Thus 
an  influx  of  consonants,  to  say  nothing  of  meanings,  tended  to 
reduce  the  lengths  of  the  tappings.  Changes  in  the  apperceptive 
consciousness  appear  to  induce  changes  in  the  motor  setting. 

O,  from  the  introspection  given  upon  the  experiments  concerned 
with  this  vowel,  was  more  easily  said  than  A,  directed  from  the 
mouth  more  horizontally,  more  of  an  object  of  the  esthetic  con- 
sciousness, and  more  associated  with  the  wind  and  water  sounds 
of  nature  than  the  preceding  one.  Consciousness  'had  more  play 
with  regard  to  O  than  A, — one  could  inspect  the  fringes  and 
return  to  the  focus,  or  maintain  disparate  foci  quite  easily  during 
its  recitation. 

The  character  of  the  long  I  was  found  to  be  considerably  more 
intense  and  forceful  than  that  of  A  or  O.  Also  the  diphthongal 
character  was  very  poorly  concealed  under  the  constant  recur- 
rence of  the  unaccented  consonant.  But  I  is  a  true  diphthong 
and  the  introspection  thus  faithfully  gives  a  prominent  place  both 
to  the  E-vanish  and  to  the  Italian  A  with  which  it  begins.  Its 
pitch  seemed  at  once  higher,  its  utterance  less  smooth,  and  the 
mouth  movement  more  conscious  than  that  of  the  preceding 
vowels;  the  drop  and  lift  of  the  lower  jaw  was  ever  consciously 
prominent.  It  was  frequently  remarked  that  this  vowel  had  very 
little  connection  with  feelings  of  personality;  the  labial  conso- 
nants had  very  much  more  to  do  with  one's  self  than  did  the  other 
consonants,  and  all  felt  much  more  intimate  than  did  any  of  the 
vowels. 

E  was  the  most  intense  of  all  the  vowels,  feelings  of  strain  at 
once  appearing;  but  it  was  also  more  easily  controlled  by  the 
muscles  of  the  vocal  apparatus  than  was  I.  Not  so  resonant  as 


ELEMENTS  OF  SPEECH  IN  RELATION  TO  POETRY         29 

the  O,  but  it  had  far  more  "color"  than  the  A.  Reference  was 
always  external,  the  word  "he"  appearing  to  be  thoroughly  ono- 
matopoetic.  The  tense  condition  of  the  mouth  during  its  utter- 
ance often  gave  a  feeling  of  weariness,  as  the  position  of  the 
cheeks,  lips  and  jaws  is  more  rigid  than  in  the  case  of  the  other 
vowels  experimented  upon. 

If  one  should  ask  how  these  four  vowels  stand  in  the  order 
of  pleasantness,  the  answer  is  that  O  was  chosen  pleasant  126 
times  out  of  240  judgments,  I  123,  E  120,  and  A  119  times.  The 
consonants  were  preferred  in  the  following  order:  R  (28  out 
of  forty  judgments),  L  27,  N  27,  V  25,  M  24,  B  24,  D  23,  Z  22, 
Ch  21,  F  20,  P  19,  W  19,  J  18  K  18,  S  18,  T  18,  Th  17,  St  17, 
H  15,  Q  15,  G  14,  and  Sh  12.  One  is  referred  in  this  connection 
to  an  article  in  the  American  Journal  of  Psychology,  1912,  by 
Louise  Roblee  and  M.  F.  Washburn,  on  the  "Affective  Values  of 
Articulate  Sounds,"  in  which  quite  similar  results  are  brought 
forth;  the  judgments  of  pleasure  and  displeasure  were  in  general 
confined  to  the  final  consonants  and  vowels,  and  many  more 
sounds  were  used  by  these  experimenters  than  we  have  employed 
in  the  above  experiments.  But  that  S  and  I  are  quite  netural  in 
character,  as  the  above-mentioned  article  indicates,  we  have  not 
found  to  be  the  case  in  our  own  work.  I  was  very  insistent,  and 
S  became  the  basis  for  more  unpleasant  judgments  in  the  trans- 
mogrifications than  any  other  single  sound  in  the  language. 

If  we  compare  the  consonants  in  the  order  of  their  pleasantness 
with  their  order  of  frequency,  as  given  in  the  introductory  para- 
graphs, it  will  be  seen  that  the  two  orders  do  not  entirely  corre- 
spond ;  nevertheless,  it  is  plain  that  the  pleasanter  of  the  sounds, 
as  found  in  the  brief  sampling  of  the  consonants,  are  those  which 
occupy  the  positions  of  higher  frequency;  had  all  of  the  con- 
sonants been  combined  with  all  the  vowels,  the  discrepancies 
might  have  been  less.  Three  factors  seem  to  militate  against 
pleasantness  in  the  pronunciation  of  a  consonant:  breathiness, 
vigorous  movement  of  the  lips,  and  the  employment  of  the  ends 
of  the  tongue  in  articulation. 

A  correlation  between  feeling  tone  and  motor  discharge  was 


30  ROBERT  CHENAULT  GIVLER 

found  in  the  following  way:  If  one  arranges  the  averages  of 
all  the  tappings  for  each  of  the  experiments  upon  any  one  of  the 
vowels  in  the  descending  order  of  motor  output,  and  places 
side  by  side  the  same  twenty-four  experiments  arranged  in  the 
ascending  or  descending  order  of  pleasantness,  it  will  be  seen 
that  in  the  case  of  the  A-experiments  that  the  more  unpleasant 
were  correlated  with  the  greater  expenditure  of  motor  energy 
and  vice  versa.  So  with  the  other  three  vowels,  the  O,  I,  and  E. 
This  is  exactly  in  line  with  what  was  found  in  the  case  of  most 
of  the  earlier  experiments  in  this  investigation.  Differences  in 
one  to  one  correspondence  appear,  however  in  the  case  of  each 
of  these  vowels:  with  the  long  A,  there  are  seven  such  corre- 
spondences ;  with  O,  there  are  eleven ;  with  I,  but  two,  and  with 
E,  five.  The  average  displacement  for  the  others  is  with  the  A, 
nearly  nine  points;  with  O,  eleven;  with  I,  eight;  and  with  E, 
nine.  It  was  also  to  be  learned  that  the  explosive  consonants 
aroused  the  motor  consciousness  more  than  the  softer  and  more 
liquid  sounds.  Besides,  those  sounds  requiring  the  more  facial 
movement  while  uttering  them  arouse  the  more  general  somatic 
activity. 

Differences  in  the  time  taken  to  recite  these  experiments  were 
not  to  be  correlated  with  either  of  the  above  factors  of  affection 
or  motor  output;  it  is  true  that  the  more  explosive  sounds  tend 
to  be  said  very  quickly,  perhaps  indeed,  because  many  of  the 
subjects  tried  to  say  them  quickly  to  get  done  with  them.  But 
the  liquids  also  went  quickly,  because  they  blended  well  together 
in  the  combinations, — indeed  the  whole  line  of  five  iambics  often 
went  like  one  ten-syllable  foot,  according  to  the  introspective 
report. 

As  typical  of  the  numerical  results  of  these  simple  vowel  and 
consonant  experiments  we  next  present  the  averages  of  the  tap- 
pings by  each  subject  together  with  the  mean  variation  and  the 
range,  and  also  the  rank  lists  for  the  above  results  in  the  case 
of  the  long  O  experiments.  It  will  be  noticed  that  most  of  the 
subjects  change  their  position  in  these  rank  lists  quite  frequently : 
this  will  not  mean  that  there  was  a  corresponding  change  in  feel- 
ing tone,  but  only  in  motor  settings  and  motor  arousal.  With  the 


ELEMENTS  OF  SPEECH  IN  RELATION  TO  POETRY 


vowel,  O,  however,  less  variations  from  average  position  occurred 
in  the  case  of  each  subject.  The  character  of  O  from  the  intro- 
spective report  is  an  interesting  corollary  to  this  fact:  O  was 
the  most  pleasant  of  the  four  vowels,  and  the  play  of  conscious- 
ness about  it  was  greatest  on  the  side  of  introspection,  but  appar- 
ently not  as  regards  the  motory  end  of  the  matter. 

The  mean  of  the  tappings  for  the  long  O  experiments  follows : 


Subject  -Bo 

A 86.2 

B 94.8 

C.  HO.O 


D. 
F. 
K. 
L. 
M. 
P. 
S. 


90.2 

90.2 

41.6 

77.6 

89.2 

126.5 

103.7 


-Ko 

A 68.1 

B 89.5 

C 103.2 


D. 

F. 
K. 
L. 
M. 
P. 
S. 


92.7 

94-8 

43-9 

71-8 

Qi.S 

116.1 

108.3 


-Ro 

A 81.2 

B 95-0 

C 109.0 

D 94.4 

F 86.6 

K 67.5 

L 68.0 

M 98.9 

P II7-3 

S 111.9 


A. 
B. 
C. 
D. 
F. 
K. 
L. 
M. 
P. 
S. 


-Cho 

84.5 

774 

105.9 

93-3 

96.8 

44-5 

74.1 

96.3 

117.8 

117.4 


-Do 

85-3 

93-6 

106.3 

84.6 

96.8 

47.6 

79-0 

91.2 

125.2 

102.4 

-Lo 
80.5 
64.6 


101.5 
87.8 

35-9 
70.9 

93-7 
117.8 

104.5 

-So 
84.2 
91.6 
109.6 
88.0 
88.9 

55-7 
72.0 


122.2 
1044 

-Sho 
75-1 


107.5 

93-7 

97-8 

49.2 

70.0 

94.0 

118.9 

118.5 


-Fo 
92.4 

79-7 

105.1 

88.4 

96.7 

51.6 

714 

91-5 

1154 


-Mo 

79-7 
84.1 
101.7 
97-8 
90.6 
30.1 
65.3 
94.0 

II5-4 
108.5 

-To 

83.3 

91.1 

105.2 

91.2 

87.4 

37-2 

74-9 

92.8 

118.4 

110.7 

-Sto 
86.1 
86.4 
105.5 
93-5 
93-8 
3I-I 
78.8 

103.4 
122.7 
118.0 


-Go 

90.5 
81.6 
105.6 
83.2 
92.6 

53-4 
62.1 

91.5 

120.8 

105.8 

-No 
65.0 
92.7 

ii'i.5 
96.6 

89.5 
25.2 
66.0 
90.7 

1 21.0 
II2.2 

-Vo 
88.1 

89.3 

101.4 
95-9 
86.7 
42.1 

77-4 

90.8 

124.4 

113.2 

-Tho 

75-8 
92.6 

IO0.2 

94-7 


39-8 
73-1 
96.0 

125-4 
II5-5 


-Ho 

88.2 

81.3 

105.1 

93-9 
92.2 

545 
62.5 

90-3 
120.4 
108.0 

-Po 

91-3 

88.7 
107.1 

98-5 

82.2 

44-0 

72.2 

92.2 

124.1 

107.4 

-Wo 

837 

95-0 

102.8 

96.3 

79-8 

44.9 

63.8 

90.7 

124.8 

113-6 

-O- 

75-0 

82.7 

106.7 

102.0 

93-8 
53-8 
58.3 
97-4 

121. 1 
120.5 


-Jo 

80.7 
80.8 

104.5 
81.2 

93-8 

44-7 

67.5 

95-0 

117.6 

109.8 

-Qo 
88.6 

834 

in. i 

90.7 

90.2 

37-3 

67.5 

98.0 

127.8 

107.6 

-Zo 

87.1 

91.8 

106.8 

96.0 

83.2 

42.8 

74.0 

91.7 

125.4 

II3-7 

-O2- 

74.6 

IOI.4 

97-4 
101.8 
95-2 
45-7 
71.8 

954 
124.2 
125.6 


ROBERT  CHENAULT  GIVLER 


The  rank  list  for  the  Mean :  La-Bo,  etc. 


Subject 

B 

D 

F 

G 

H       J 

K 

L 

M 

N 

P 

Q 

A  

c 

e 

f 

e 

d        c 

b 

d 

c 

h 

e 

d 

B 

f 

c 

c 

c        d 

d 

c 

d 

f 

d 

c 

c  

i 

i 

i 

h 

h        h 

h 

h 

h 

h 

i 

D  

e 

d 

d 

d 

s       e 

f 

h 

•f 

F  

f 

K 

K 

g 

f        f 

g 

e 

e 

d 

c 

e 

K  

a 

a 

a 

a 

a        a 

a 

a 

a 

n 

a 

a 

L  

b 

b 

b 

b 

b        b 

c 

b 

b 

c 

b 

b 

M  

d 

c 

e 

f 

e       g 

e 

f 

f 

e 

f 

g 

P  

J 

J 

J 

J 

J        J 

J 

J 

J 

j 

J 

J 

S  

h 

h 

h 

i 

i        i 

i 

i 

i 

i 

i 

h 

Subject 

R 

S 

T 

V 

W      Z 

OH 

SH 

ST    TH 

0 

02 

A  

c 

c 

c 

d 

d       d 

d 

c 

c 

c 

c 

c 

B  

f 

R 

e 

e 

f       f 

c 

d 

d 

e 

d 

C  

h 

i 

h 

h 

h        h 

.    h 

h 

h 

h 

h 

D  

e 

d 

f 

g 

g       g 

e 

e 

e 

f 

g 

h 

F  

d 

f 

d 

c 

c       c 

g 

g 

d 

d 

e 

d 

K  

a 

a 

a 

a 

a        a 

a 

a 

a 

a 

a 

a 

L  

b 

b 

b 

b 

b       b 

b 

b 

b 

b 

b 

b 

M  

g 

e 

•g 

f 

e        e 

f 

f 

g 

K 

f 

e 

P  

J 

J 

} 

J 

J        J 

J 

J 

J 

J 

J 

i 

S  

i 

h 

i 

i 

i        i 

i 

i 

i 

i 

i 

) 

The  mean 

variations 

for  these  experiments 

:  La-Bo,  etc. 

Subject 

Bo 

Do 

Fo 

Go 

Ho 

Jo 

Ko 

Lo 

A  

4.2 

7-1 

3-7 

5-0 

3-5 

3-6 

6-7 

2.6 

B  

4.6 

3,2 

3-1 

3-  1 

3-8 

4-8 

7-2 

4-7 

C  

3-7 

2.4 

2A 

2.4 

3-1 

3.3 

2.7 

2.8 

D  

2.1 

5-6 

2.6 

5-2 

1.8 

5-o 

2.8 

2.2 

F  

3-8 

3-3 

3-0 

3-3 

5-6 

4-3 

34 

5-5 

K  

4.1 

4-9 

4.2 

3-4 

4-r 

4.0 

2.9 

4-8 

L  

2.8 

2.5 

4,1 

4.8 

5.5 

4-5 

5.6 

3-8 

M  

3-0 

1.6 

2.8 

2.2 

2.3 

2.4 

2.4 

34 

P  

1.9 

2.1 

5-0 

3-0 

2.2 

34 

3-8 

3-8 

Subject 

Mo 

No 

Po 

Qo 

Ro 

So 

To 

Vo 

A  

3-3 

4.0 

2.8 

4-9 

5.7 

3-2 

4-8 

5-3 

B  

4-0 

4-3 

7-3 

5-2 

4-5 

5-8 

5-1 

7-0 

C  

3-1 

2.8 

3-2 

2.9 

2.8 

3-3 

3-2 

2.4 

D  

1.4 

2.2 

2.9 

3.2 

2-5 

3-6 

1.8 

3-9 

F  

3-2 

3-8 

6.2 

3-9 

3-6 

3-6 

3-9 

3-6 

K.  

44 

3-7 

3-8 

4-8 

5-6 

4-3 

54 

44 

L  

3-5 

4.n 

2-3 

4-1 

2.7 

3-1 

6.0 

3-8 

M  

2.4 

4.2 

3-3 

4.0 

2.6 

2.9 

34 

4.2 

P  

3-8 

3-7 

5-9 

2.5 

1.8 

24 

2.5 

2.5 

S  

2.6 

1-7 

4-0 

2.6 

2.2 

4-6 

4.1 

2-5 

Subject 

Wo 

Zo 

Cho 

Sho 

Sto 

Tho 

0 

02 

A  

4-1 

4-1 

5-4 

7-1 

5-2 

4.1 

4.2 

4.1 

B  

4-8 

5-0 

4.1 

5-5 

6.0 

5-8 

4-1 

4.6 

C  

27 

3-7 

3-0 

3-3 

2.5 

2.2 

3-0 

2.8 

D  

2.8 

3-9 

2.1 

1.6 

2.6 

3-5 

1.6 

2.6 

F  

3-0 

4.0 

3-2 

2-7 

2.9 

4-7 

1.9 

3-8 

K.  

4-2 

5-8 

5-9 

3.2 

4-7 

5-2 

4-7 

S-i 

L  

4.3 

4-3 

4.8 

5-6 

4-5 

54 

5-7 

5-8 

M  

2.6 

3-2 

3-6 

3-9 

2.8 

5-5 

2.7 

3-7 

P  

2.3 

I'.2 

2.O 

2.9 

2.9 

1.2 

2.1 

1.9 

S  

3-1 

2.7 

3-3 

3-i 

4.1 

44 

3-6 

2-9 

The  rank  list  for  these  mean  variations :  La-Bo,  etc. 


Subject  

B 

D 

F 

G 

H 

J 

K 

L       M 

N 

P 

Q 

A  

h 

j 

i 

f 

e 

h 

b        f 

g 

b 

i 

B  

i 

e 

e 

e 

g 

i 

i 

h        j 

j 

j 

j 

C  

e 

c 

a 

b 

e 

c 

b 

c       d 

c 

d 

c 

D  

b 

i 

b 

J 

a 

J 

c 

a       a 

b 

c 

d 

F.  

£ 

f 

d 

f 

J 

g 

f 

J        e 

f 

i 

e 

K.  

g 

h 

i 

K 

h 

e 

i        i 

e 

f 

h 

L  

c 

d 

h 

h 

i 

h 

J 

f        g 

h 

a 

g 

M  

d 

a 

c 

a 

c 

b 

a 

d       b 

i 

e 

f 

P  

a 

b 

1 

c 

b 

d 

g 

e       h 

d 

h 

a 

S  

J 

g 

f 

d 

d 

a 

d 

g       c 

a 

g 

b 

Subject 

R 

S 

T 

V 

W 

Z 

OH 

SH    ST 

TH 

O 

O2 

A  

j 

d 

K 

i 

g 

g 

j 

i        i 

g 

d 

h 

B  

h 

1 

h 

i 

1 

h 

h 

i        -s 

i 

j 

C  

f 

e 

c 

a 

c 

c 

f 

a        c 

d 

b 

e 

D.  

f 

a 

f 

d 

b 

a 

b        b 

e 

c 

a 

F  

g 

K 

e 

d 

e 

f 

b 

e        d 

f 

f 

b 

K.  

i 

h 

i 

h 

h 

J 

e 

h        j 

J 

g 

i 

L  

e 

c 

J 

e 

i 

i 

i 

g       h 

h 

h 

J 

M  

d 

b 

d 

g 

b 

e 

g 

d        f 

c 

i 

d 

p  

a 

a 

h 

b 

a 

a 

c 

c       a 

a 

a 

c 

S  

b 

i 

f 

c 

f 

d 

d 

f        e 

b 

e 

f 

The 

rank 

list 

for  the  ranges  : 

La-Bo,  etc. 

Subject 

Bo 

Do 

Fo 

Go 

Ho 

Jo 

Ko 

Lo 

A  

20 

32 

21 

26 

16 

16 

35 

12 

B  

24 

114 

18 

16 

17 

20 

27 

2.S 

C  

18 

i.S 

9 

14 

15 

17 

16 

H 

D  

in 

28 

12 

30 

12 

22 

20 

II 

F  

18 

18 

Ifi 

22 

23 

18 

21 

28 

K  

18 

22 

16 

19 

18 

23 

29 

21 

L.    

IS 

13 

19 

2O 

21 

IIQ 

25 

17 

M  , 

14 

9 

13 

IO 

IO 

II 

16 

16 

P  

8 

IO 

2O 

12 

II 

21 

12 

22 

S  

19 

22 

24 

18 

2O 

8 

I'S 

26 

Subject 

Mo 

No 

Po 

Qo 

Ro 

So 

To 

Vo 

A  , 

23 

23 

15 

23 

22 

15 

2Q 

z$ 

B  

30 

21 

26 

28 

23 

24 

39 

28 

C  

,ni 

IS 

14 

IS 

III 

23 

17 

14 

D  

9 

12 

i'3 

16 

13 

17 

12 

19 

F  , 

241 

19 

28 

i'7 

24 

H3 

20 

18 

K  

21 

2O 

19 

29 

2O 

25 

25 

27 

L  , 

18 

16 

21 

24 

14 

18 

27 

22 

M  

12 

22 

16 

2Ii 

211 

12 

22 

20 

P  

19 

18 

3» 

HI 

12 

21 

H 

17 

S  , 

IO 

9 

37 

20 

9 

16 

18 

12 

Subject 

Wo 

Zo 

Cho 

Sho 

Sto    Tho 

O 

02 

A  

25 

20 

19 

31 

25 

19 

21 

22 

B  

32 

2* 

23 

25 

30 

30 

18 

26 

C  

19 

15 

13 

i'3 

'II 

II 

17 

12 

D  

13 

12 

in 

7 

15 

17 

7 

IO 

F  

m 

18 

15 

17 

20 

24 

n  r 

17 

K.    

28 

28 

26 

12 

18 

22 

21 

23 

L.    

22 

16 

21 

37 

22 

18 

27 

18 

M  

14 

17 

18 

19 

21 

23 

19 

21 

P  

18 

7 

9 

18 

19 

7 

16 

II 

S  

20 

14 

14 

n 

24 

33 

20 

2O 

34  ROBERT  CHENAULT  GIVLER 

The  rank  list  for  the  ranges :     La-Bo,  etc. 

Subject  BDFGHJKLMNPQ 

A i        j         i        i        e        c        j         b        h  j  c  g 

B j        d        f        d        f        g        h        h        j  h  g  i 

C e        e        a        c        d        d        d       c        c  c  b  b 

D b        i        b        j         c        i        e        a     -a  b  a  c 

F f        f        d        h        j         e        f        j        i  f  i  d 

K g        h        e        f        g       j         i        f        g  g  e  j 

L. dcggi        fgeedfh 

M c        a        c        a        a        b        c        d        d  i  d  e 

P a       b        h        b        b        h        a        g        f  e  j  a 

S h       g       j        e       h       a       b       i        a  a  h  f 


Subject  R  S  T 

A h  c  i  h  h  h  g  i  i  e  i  h 

B i  i  j  j  j  i  i  h  j  i  e  j 

C b  h  c  b  e  d  c  d  a  b  d  c 

D d  e  a  e  b  b  b  a  b  c  a  a 

F j  b  e  d  a  g  e  e  e  h  b  d 

K f  j  g  i  i  j  j  c  c  f  h  i 

L e  f  h  g  g  e  h  j  g  d  j  e 

M gaffcffgfgfg 

P c  g  b  c  d  a  a  f  d  a  c  b 

S a  d  d  a  f  c  d  b  h  j  g  f 

In  measuring  the  ranges,  it  often  happened  that  two  or  three 
subjects  could  have  had  the  same  position;  re-measuring,  how- 
ever, or  alloting  to  the  subjects  that  position  which  they  had 
previously  tended  to  maintain,  obviated  the  difficulty;  for  ex- 
ample, where  subjects  A.  and  B.  were  equally  set  for  the  position 
C  in  the  rank  lists,  but  had  previously  maintained  positions  B  and 
C  respectively,  we  assigned  to  them  positions  B  and  C,  in  order 
both  to  have  ten  positions,  and  to  give  each  of  them  the  benefit 
of  the  doubt.  With  differences  of  tenths  of  a  millimeter  as 
the  basis  for  many  of  the  correlations,  it  was  not  always  easy 
to  determine  the  exact  status  of  affairs  for  any  one  subject  with 
respect  to  another  better  than  by  the  above  method. 

If  one  is  again  asked  what  changes  of  position  in  the  rank 
list  for  the  Mean  denote,  or  even  what  changes  in  the  average 
of  the  tappings  denote,  we  are  somewhat  at  loss  to  give  a  'fully 
satisfactory  answer;  it  is  not  due  to  a  change  in  feeling  tone 
so  much  as  it  is  due  to  various  manifestations  of  the  motor  con- 
sciousness during  the  continuance  of  the  same  feeling  state. 
As  was  previously  noticed  in  the  first  year's  work,  it  is  usually 


ELEMENTS  OF  SPEECH  IN  RELATION  TO  POETRY         35 

quite  a  while  after  the  onset  of  a  new  feeling  state,  that  a  change 
takes  place  in  the  motor  manifestations.  And  insofar  as  we 
correlate  with  the  introspective  report  as  a  basis,  we  find  that 
the  mean  variation  of  the  tappings  made  during  pleasant  states 
is  greater  than  the  mean  variation  of  the  tappings  made  during 
unpleasant  or  neutral  states;  thus  pleasure  is  manifested,  at 
least  in  this  experiment,  by  more  varied  expressive  means  than 
are  the  other  affective  states  of  consciousness.  The  introspection 
in  connection  with  the  pleasant  experiments  is  richer,  the  asso- 
ciative functions  are  more  operative,  and  the  general  bodily 
and  mental  condition  is  more  indicative  of  ever  new  and  varied 
manifestations  in  those  states  in  which  fine  esthetic  feelings  are 
present  than  in  those  which  appear  to  indicate  the  presence  of 
cloggings,  inhibitions  and  mutually  antagonistic  impulses. 

Graphing  these  experiments  revealed  the  following  characteris- 
tic differences  between  the  vowels : 

The  ascending  order  of  motor  output  followed  the  series  as 
presented,  A,  O,  I  and  E.  This  may,  however,  be  due  to  practise 
alone.  But  within  any  group  of  experiments  other  differences 
are  quite  significant,  especially  if  compared  with  the  introspec- 
tion as  given  above;  the  O-graphs  showed  the  least  scatter,  the 
E  and  I  the  most.  E  and  I  also  march  straight  across  the  page, 
while  A  shows  an  "Anregung"  incessantly  throughout  the  series. 
E  and  I  are  also  spoken  in  a  shorter  time  than  are  O  and  A,  and 
besides,  the  O-graphs  are  all  indicative  of  the  fact  that  this 
vowel  was  spoken  in  more  nearly  the  same  time  even  though 
preceded  by  the  various  consonants  than  were  the  E  and  I. 
Here,  in  the  case  of  O,  steadiness  of  motor  discharge,  as  evi- 
denced by  the  fact  that  the  vowel  tends  to  swallow  the  consonants 
which  precede  it,  is  correlated  with  steadiness  and  evenness  of  in- 
trospectional  content  and  attitude;  with  E  and  I,  on  the  other 
hand,  the  exact  opposite  is  the  case.  The  number  of  factors 
involved  is  many,  and  the  final  result  may  perhaps  be  tentatively 
stated  as  follows:  Quickness  of  utterance  is  correlated  with 
greater  motor  output ;  strain  in  the  vocal  apparatus  with  uneven- 
ness  of  motor  output;  ease  with  which  the  vowel  is  spoken 


36  ROBERT  CHENAULT  GIVLER 

dominating  the  strain-effect  of  the  consonants;  and  a  negative 
correlation  between  unevenness  (though  unfelt)  in  the  motor 
output,  and  also  amount  of  such  output  and  the  pleasantness  as 
aroused  in  the  introspectional  conscious  content. 

From  a  different  graphing  of  these  experiments,  four  in  a 
group,  on  the  basis  of  consonants  preceding  the  vowels,  where 
in  each  group  of  four  drawings,  one  finds  those  graphs  together 
which  show  the  differences  between  the  four  vowels  under  the 
influence  of  the  same  initial  accented  consonant,  we  found  that 
in  nearly  all  the  cases  the  A  graph  is  the  lowest,  frequently  very 
much  the  lowest,  while  again,  the  O,  E,  and  I  graphs  exchange 
positions  of  height  and  extent  to  some  considerable  degree.  But 
this  is  quite  natural, — if  the  O  had  been  by  far  the  pleasantest 
vowel,  or  the  R  by  far  the  pleasantest  consonant,  we  might  have 
expected  the  R  and  O  graphs  to  appear  unique  and  different 
from  the  others,  but  the  vowels  are  almost  equally  pleasant,  a 
difference  of  but  seven  judgments  of  agreeability  separating  the 
O  and  the  A  (the  extremes),  and  the  consonants  grade  very 
gently  from  the  most  to  the  least  agreeable.  So  it  is  perhaps 
correct  to  say  that  the  different  graphs  represent  the  matter  not 
so  much  from  the  vowel  side  as  from  the  consonant  side  when 
we  compare  the  graphs  for  any  one  vowel  together  but  that 
comparing  one  of  these  large  groups  with  another,  we  have  vowel 
differences  rather  than  consonant  differences  before  us. 

The  introspection  revealed  the  fact  that  entirely  different  states 
of  mind  were  aroused  according  to  changes  in  the  direction  of 
the  attention;  instructions  were  given  to  neglect  the  physical 
sensations  and  to  think  of  the  sounds  as  much  as  possible,  in 
order  to  have  constant  conditions  for  all  the  subjects.  But  this 
does  not  seem  to  have  worked  very  satisfactorily  in  many  of  the 
above  cases;  frequently,  indeed,  mixed  feelings  were  reported; 
the  subjects  would  say :  "The  sounds  are  not  unpleasant,  but  I 
do  not  like  to  say  them" ;  or,  "The  sounds  as  heard  are  all  right, 
but  the  everlasting  mouth  movement  is  exceedingly  hard  to  keep 
out  of  the  focus  of  consciousness."  Absolute  restraint  was  im- 
possible ;  it  would  also  have  been  quite  unwise,  for  we  were  study- 


ELEMENTS  OF  SPEECH  IN  RELATION  TO  POETRY         37 

ing  the  motor  as  well  as  the  introspective  consciousness  and 
to  find  out  just  what  happened  in  apparent  conflicts  between  the 
two,  especially  on  the  side  of  the  affections,  was  considered  just  as 
valuable  as  anything  else.  It  was  thought  better  to  let  things 
take  pretty  much  their  own  course  in  the  matter  rather  than  to 
be  too  severely  restrictive.  But  that  the  introspection  was  not 
universally  given  on  the  same  elements  in  consciousness  is  at  once 
evident;  whether  it  could  be,  whether  one  can  abstract  one  ele- 
ment and  keep  it  abstracted  throughout  the  series  is  very  doubt- 
ful indeed.  And  yet,  when  we  compare  the  amounts  of  motor 
discharge  for  the  different  subjects,  the  question  comes  up: 
"What  was  it  that  was  called  pleasant  or  unpleasant?"  Well,  the 
only  thing  to  say  is  that  the  whole  experience  extended  toward  this 
or  that  type  of  affective  tone,  and  that  is  about  all  that  can  be  said. 
And  if  that  is  insufficient,  then  some  other  method  must  be  de- 
vised to  experiment  upon  these  simple  vowel  and  consonant 
combinations  than  we  have  employed.  One  will  also  notice  that 
the  subjects  talked  about  some  of  the  combinations  having  a 
higher  pitch  than  others, — unless  the  pitch  was  changed,  sub- 
consciously, of  course,  to  relieve  the  uniformity,  this  is  quite 
incomprehensible;  surely  the  vowel  A  does  not  take  a  different 
pitch  in  connection  with  some  of  the  consonants  than  it  does  with 
the  others,  and  if  it  seemed  to,  it  is  not  unlikely  that  this  was 
an  illusion  due  to  the  greater  intensity  required  to  enunciate  the 
explosive  consonants.  And  as  there  was  no  uniformity  on  the 
judgments  of  pitch,  even  among  the  two  or  three  subjects  who 
made  them,  it  is  hardly  possible  that  the  pitch  judgments  indi- 
cate anything  objective. 

One  final  point  also  needs  to  be  emphasized ;  which  is  that  the 
tendency  to  make  words  out  of  these  meaningless  experiments 
was  super-strong  with  nearly  all  of  the  subjects.  As  one  subject 
said  in  regard  to  the  transmogrifications :  ''It  tantalizes  me 
dreadfully  because  the  words  I  get  out  of  them  have  such  dis- 
junctive meanings."  So  that  some  severe  critic  might  call  this 
whole  work,  "An  experiment  in  the  delayed  associations  of  mis- 
spelled words."  But  following  such  caustic  criticism,  let  such 


38  ROBERT  CHENAULT  GIVLER 

a  person  suggest  some  better  way  than  we  have  used  to  experi- 
ment on  the  psycho-physiologies  of  the  sounds  of  the  language, 
and  thus  help  us  out  of  the  dilemma.  We  admit  right  at  the 
start  that  the  whole  realm  of  psychological  esthetics  seems  to  be 
constituted  chiefly  by  its  difficulties. 

SIMPLE  EXPERIMENTS  CONSTRUCTED  FROM  THE 

TABULATIONS  OF  SOUND  FREQUENCY  IN 

ENGLISH  POETRY 

In  connection  with  the  next  two  sets  of  experiments  reference 
must  be  made  to  the  previously  mentioned  tabulations  of  sound 
frequency  in  the  poets.  If  one  arranges  these  percentages  in 
order  of  magnitude  for  each  one  of  the  poets,  it  will  be  noticed 
that  certain  sounds  are  almost  equally  prominent  for  all  of 
them,  especially  in  the  unaccented  lists;  and  here  we  refer  to 
the  short  U,  I  and  A.  Of  the  accented  sounds,  the  consonants, 
rather  than  the  vowels  appear  to  be  common  property,  inasmuch 
as  R,  T,  D,  L,  S  and  M  usually  stand  at  the  head  of  the  lists. 

The  next  twenty-seven  experiments  were  devised  to  show  the 
effect  of  the  most  prominent  of  these  accented  and  unaccented 
sounds.  They  nearly  all  contain  two  unaccented  and  three  ac- 
cented letters.  But  these  combinations,  unlike  those  which  have 
hitherto  been  employed,  usually  end  with  a  consonant,  L,  N,  D 
and  T  predominating.  Thus  the  body  of  sound  produced  by  their 
utterance  is  something  more  solid  than  we  have  had  before ;  and 
the  organs  of  articulation  exercise  more  control  than  they  did  in 
the  case  of  the  long,  open  vowels. 

No  single  poet's  preference  for  certain  sounds  is  especially 
represented  in  these  combinations;  the  whole  twenty-seven  of 
them  merely  exhibit  the  most  used  accented  and  unaccented 
sounds  of  English  poetry  "ueberhaupt."  The  introspections  for 
these  experiments,  which  were  given  in  the  following  order,  is 
of  interest : 

I.  Ne-rol.  (Iambic  foot,  vowels  both  short;  repeated  five 
times  in  succession  for  each  of  the  five  groups.) 

Implies  the  joys  of  rustic  work  and  pleasure;  visual  imagery  of  the  fields  hi 
summer.  Soft  and  musical;  suggests  rapidity  of  movement;  imagery  of  some 


ELEMENTS  OF  SPEECH  IN  RELATION  TO  POETRY         39 

May  morning.  Soft,  drawn-out  thing;  not  very  active  nor  deep,  but  it  has 
body.  Very  musical  and  easy  to  say;  sounds  like  the  ringing  of  a  bell; 
slightly  monotonous,  but  not  dull. 

II.  Un-ral(e).      (As  above,  metrically  and  in  groups;  short 
"u,"  long  "a".) 

"R"  the  best  thing  about  it;  seems  slightly  inharmonious  and  perturbing; 
no  definite  imagery.  Not  energetic;  couldn't  keep  the  word  "unreal"  out  of 
mind.  "Ral"  gives  a  feeling  of  contraction;  seems  inefficient;  thinks  of  the 
pattering  of  hail  or  of  big  water  drops. 

III.  Id-rel.     (Vowels  both  short.) 

Sounds  have  a  bell-like  quality;  seems  to  refer  to  some  celebration.  Hard 
to  keep  the  two  syllables  apart.  Became  "id-well,"  "did-well";  dislikes  the 
jump  from  the  first  syllable  to  the  second.  Musical,  hopeful  sound;  keeps 
ascending  in  pitch. 

IV.  Ri-tin(e).     (First  "i"  short;  second,  long.) 

Visualizes  self  on  the  sea  shore  on  a  warm,  summer  day;  feels  the  heat 
and  his  own  body  distinctly.  Makes  him  frown.  Seems  narrowly  concen- 
trated in  space.  Musical ;  in  major  key ;  encouraging ;  calls  up  the  word 
"time."  Emphatic,  but  monotonous;  gets  nowhere. 

V.  Tu-lin.  (Both  vowels  short.) 

Energetic  and  poetic;  visualized  the  sea;  heard  the  wind  and  the  sound 
of  the  breakers.  Very  easy  and  pretty  sound  to  make ;  suggests  a  light, 
fleet  movement.  Seems  rapid ;  "tu"  is  light ;  "lin"  heavy ;  good  combination 
of  sounds ;  very  easy  to  coordinate  finger  and  voice.  Soothing  and  quieting ; 
a  lullaby;  the  alternation  of  the  vowels  is  charming. 

VI.  Ti-ren.     (Both  vowels  short.) 

Sharp  and  concise  and  easy  to  produce;  but  the  "n"  seems  to  negate  the 
expected  climactic  character  of  it.  The  superficial  gloominess  of  a  rainy, 
indoor  day  implied;  not  very  "deep"  sound;  visual  imagery  dark  gray. 
Couldn't  keep  track  of  the  counting.  Forceful  sound;  calls  up  the  words 
'"to  arms !" 

VII.  Ti-rel.     (Vowels  both  short.) 

Bell-like ;  musical  and  melodious ;  implies  recreation ;  visualizes  a  country 
dance  in  the  moonlight;  slightly  erotic.  Energetic  and  speedy;  runs  together 
well ;  suggests  the  full  pleasure  of  animal  spirits.  Just  a  happy  little  fool's 
song;  jolly.  Good  lyric  poetry;  "It  may  be  flip,  but  never  mind";  the 
sounds  run  together  beautifully.  Exciting  and  exhilarating;  feels  the  pitch 
to  be  very  high;  brings  a  joyful  and  exuberant  feeling. 

VIII.  Un-dol(e).     (First  vowel  short;  second,  long.) 

Has  distinct  musical  quality,  but  the  tone  is  sad  and  mournful.  Romantic, 
but  doleful ;  recalls  the  "Niebelungen  Lied" ;  implies  the  pathetic  fallacy. 
Quiet  and  sombre;  calls  up  the  tolling  of  a  bell;  exceedingly  passive  thing. 
Means  a  mild  lament,  or  self-pity;  the  nasal  sensations  almost  become 
unpleasant. 


40  ROBERT  CHENAULT  GIVLER 

IX.  A-ren(e).     (Short  "a,"  long  "e.") 

Almost  meaningless  and  nonsensical ;  seems  to  be  calling  someone  by 
name.  Not  poetic;  thought  of  things  colloquial.  More  energetic  than 
"un-dol."  Very  uninteresting  and  commonplace;  "ri-tin"  was  intellectual; 
this  is  stupid.  Has  a  romantic  quality ;  seems  like  some  amorous  declaration  ; 
musical.  A  small  sound ;  it  is  minor  music ;  makes  one  quiet  and  thoughtful ; 
might  arouse  pity  and  sympathy. 

X.  Ri-nad(e).     ("I"  short,  "a"  long.) 

Something  mournful  about  it;  but  not  much  to  it.  Induces  a  slow,  pas- 
sive state.  Seems  to  stay  way  back  in  the  mouth  cavity;  can't  raise  the 
pitch  enough  to  make  it  effective.  Seems  contracted  and  nasal;  has  no  life 
or  activity  to  it ;  too  inward.  Emphatic ;  almost  a  battle  cry ;  very  dignified 
sort  of  a  thing. 

XL  Un-rin.     (Both  vowels  short.) 

Emphatic;  the  rhythm  is  easy  and  regular,  but  the  nasal  quality  is  not 
enjoyable.  Something  hopeless  about  it;  seems  like  a  cry;  Shelley's  poetry 
came  to  mind. 

XII.  Ti-ra.     (First  vowel  short;  second,  long.) 

Thinks  of  something  like  political  excitement;  "sisjboom-bah-rah"  and 
Roosevelt  prominently  in  mind.  "It's  just  some  conversation."  Nice  and 
quick;  happy  and  joyous;  expansive;  suggested  "hooray."  Implies  jubilant 
and  exultant  action. 

XIII.  Ni-dal.     (Both  vowels  short.) 

Reminds  of  very  fine  and  pleasing  music.  Active  and  energetic;  suggests 
the  military.  Calls  to  mind  the  girls  of  Biskra  in  the  street  of  Ouled  Nail. 
"It  tumbles  out  of  the  mouth  before  you  want  it  to";  implies  following 
the  line  of  least  resistance.  Something  important  and  also  impatient  about  it; 
implies  hurrying. 

XIV.  Ri-leet.     (Short  "i.") 

Like  a  bird  song;  bright  and  vivacious.  Not  very  deep,  but  joyous; 
images  a  woodland  scene  in  the  summer;  birds  and  squirrels  plentiful. 
Feels  hurried ;  thinks  of  the  song  of  a  lark.  Thought  of  "relief,"  "rillet,"  etc. 
Very  dainty,  light  and  springy;  something  bright  and  feminine  about  it. 

XV.  Ti-reen.     ("I"  short.) 

Thinks  of  the  mightiness  of  nature ;  wild  gray  ocean  and  sea  gulls  imaged. 
The  explosive  quality  of  "ti"  well  counterbalanced  by  "reeri";  it's  like  a 
ditty  sung  to  oneself.  This  is  romantic  and  "eulogistic";  might  be  a  love 
sonnet,  or  some  manifestation  of  devotion. 

XVI.  Ri-nel.     (Both  vowels  short.) 

Has  a  bell-like  quality;  "rin"  is  subjective;  "el"  objective.  Like  a  bell; 
tranquil,  but  not  solemn.  Seems  to  swell  in  volume  as  it  proceeds;  good 
sound  with  which  to  call  any  one.  Something  personal,  fatalistic,  and  strong 
about  it. 


ELEMENTS  OF  SPEECH  IN  RELATION  TO  POETRY         41 

• 

XVII.  Ni-lur.     (Short  "i";  "u"  as  in  "fur.") 

Seems  deceitful,  and  slippery;  too  smooth  to  have  any  body  to  it.  Implies 
a  Byronic  despair;  visual  imagery  of  a  gray  cold  autumn  sky.  Difficult  to 
say;  gets  way  up  in  his  nose;  suggests  peevishness;  wanted  to  prolong  the 
"lur"  so  as  to  get  a  firmer  hold  on  the  "ni."  Hard  to  say;  lacks  body;  felt 
almost  tongue-tied.  Calls  to  mind  some  foolish  person,  perpetually  grinning ; 
mouth  sensations  disagreeable.  Very  emotional;  an  intoxicating  riot  of 
sounds ;  full  of  color. 

XVIII.  Ri-dev.     (Both  vowels  short.) 

Very  peculiar;  thought  of  a  hot,  summer  day;  also  some  fiery,  physically 
exciting  passion  obtruded.  Unusual  sound;  "devil"  the  only  thing  that  came 
to  mind.  Dramatic;  alluring;  wanted  to  make  it  impressive;  something 
funereal  about  it.  'Has  resonance,  but  gives  a  drawn-in,  contracted  feeling. 
Funereal  and  mournful  thing;  yet  has  musical  quality  and  fascinates  one. 

XIX.  Ni-rees.     (Short  "i.") 

Very  musical;  sounds  like  whistling.  Feels  the  "s"  stops  one  short;  like 
putting  on  the  brakes  suddenly  while  driving  an  auto.  Mouth  movement 
seems  delicate;  tends  to  fuse  into  "nireesnirees,'*  etc.,  without  stopping 
between  the  syllables ;  like  singing  a  little  tune  to  himself ;  feels  contemplative. 
Sort  of  a  "love  motif";  imagery  of  a  woodland  scene,  with  birds  and  soft, 
quiet  places.  "S"  softens,  hushes,  smoothes;  very  light  and  dainty  sounds; 
"s"  also  seems  like  spreading  something  over  a  broad  surface.  Foreign 
sound;  slightly  wistful;  yet  gives  a  feeling  that  something  is  inevitable. 
Quieting,  slightly  monotonous  sound;  thought  it  descriptive  of  the  waves 
on  the  sea  shore. 

XX.  Un-reen.     (Short  "u.") 

Something  profound  about  it;  a  sad,  unsatisfied  cry,  either  sexual  or 
spiritual.  Sounds  like  a  call  for  help;  or  else  it  is  some  exposition  of  an 
important  theme.  Doesn't  allow  one  to  expand;  a  climax  foreshadowed, 
but  not  reached.  Something  romantic,  supplicating  and  pathetic  about  it. 

XXI.  Ni-ral.     (First  vowel  long,  last  vowel  short.) 

Imaged  a  big  country  fair,  where  everybody  was  having  the  time  of  his 
life;  "ni-raP  is  everybody  calling  everybody  else.  Called  up  visual  image 
of  Millet's  "The  Lark."  Makes  him  keep  his  mouth  open  all  the  time;  gives 
a  cold  feeling  all  over;  the  "i"  seems  like  something  pointed;  almost 
deprecative. 

XXII.  Thi-ra.    ("Th"  sonant ;  "i"  short ;  "a"  long. ) 

Has  much  quality,  but  the  lisping  character  of  the  "th"  almost  killed  the 
feeling  tone.  The  syllables  do  not  seem  to  belong  together;  "th"  irritates; 
seems  like  a  lover's  lisp.  Peculiar  mixture  of  sounds :  "th"  always  repul- 
sive; "ra"  very  fine;  almost  a  case  of  mixed  feelings.  Difficult  to  say; 
seems  like  a  scraping  movement  along  the  ground.  "A  horrid,  tongue-tied 
lisp." 


42  ROBERT  CHENAULT  GIVLER 

XXIII.  Ne-mal.     ("E"  long;  "a"  short.) 

Implies  a  superficial  pessimism  or  complaint.  Tends  to  become  "nemel"  and 
"nemalne"  (trochaic) ;  seems  to  be  just  a  matter  of  daily  conversation, 
buying  and  selling,  and  the  like.  Insistent  and  affirmative;  "ne"  is  the  dis- 
turbing factor.  Implies  maliciousness,  anger  or  irritation;  it  climaxes  into  a 
veritable  fury. 

XXIV.  Ri-naz(e).     ("I"  short;  "a"  long.) 

Very  subjective  thing;  "az"  makes  it  so  inward.  Slightly  energetic; 
imagery  of  a  rainy  day  in  the  country.  Slow,  monotonous,  sombre,  deaden- 
ing ;  demands  much  attention  to  keep  saying  it.  Smooth,  but  lacks  body ; 
like  the  voice  of  a  mediocre  clergyman  giving  a  nice,  homely  sermon.  Quiet- 
ing, harmonious  quality  to  it;  implies  the  pleasant  acceptance  of  a  situation, 
suited  to  one's  abilities.  It  means  homage  to  some  Oriental  monarch  whose 
name  is  "Rinaz."  A  lullaby ;  something  almost  hypnotic  about  it ;  fascinating. 

XXV.  Rolen.      ("O"  long;  "e"  short.) 

Expresses  activity,  but  a  superficial  kind.  Active,  and  quick;  rolls  right 
along.  Clear  cut,  vigorous  and  manly  sound;  personal  reference  to  it. 
"Rol"  should  have  the  accent;  hence  it  shuts  off  the  effect  of  the  intended 
iambic.  Rather  matter  of  fact  and  unimaginative.  Rather  strong  and 
vigorous;  tendency  to  anticipate  the  accent  with  the  finger.  Pleasant  alter- 
nations of  the  vowels;  "en"  brings  one  right  up  standing. 

XXVI.  Thi-nal.     (Both  vowels  short.)     (Th  surd.) 

"Th"  is  the  disagreeable  part  of  it;  something  remorseful  about  it.  Gives 
a  blurred  effect;  the  whole  things  seems  to  lisp.  "Th"  ruins  the  otherwise 
pleasant  effect  of  "nal."  Easy  to  say,  but  the  "nal"  is  too  nasal;  it  whines; 
reverberations  of  the  sound  felt  throughout  the  face.  An  unimaginative, 
practical,  downright  statement  of  fact.  Harsh,  complaining,  and  unsatis- 
fying; the  pitch  is  too  high  to  be  pleasant. 

XXVII.  Ri-neen.     (Short  "i,"  long  "e.") 

Too  nasal  to  be  musical;  refers  to  some  one  other  than  himself.  Rather 
tiresome  work;  thought  of  rowing  fast  and  hard  for  no  particular  purpose. 
Too  nasal;  the  final  "n"  is  the  worst  part  of  it;  not  so  mature  a  sound  as 
"rolen."  "Neen"  is  cold  and  hollow;  too  low  in  pitch;  makes  one  short  of 
breath  and  demands  great  depth  of  voice;  "ri"  is  by  itself  quite  pleasant. 

Even  from  these  fragments  of  the  introspection  given  in  con- 
nection with  the  above  experiments,  it  is  plainly  seen  that  the 
responses  of  the  subjects  to  the  material  indicate  that  they  had 
"gotten  into"  the  business  of  introspection  better  than  ever  be- 
fore. Much  of  it,  is  of  course  not  pure  introspection;  but  the 
instructions  were  only :  "give  me  what  is  in  your  mind  after  you 
have  recited  this  combination  twenty-five  times."  In  describing 
the  effect  of  these  sound  combinations,  then,  if  to  say,  "it  is  like 


ELEMENTS  OF  SPEECH  IN  RELATION  TO  POETRY         43 

this  or  that"  may  not  be  introspection,  yet  it  did  not  seem  possible 
to  obtain  any  other  introspective  results  than  the  above.  And  yet, 
in  the  light  of  future  experimentation,  just  this  kind  of  answers 
upon  the  effect  of  the  sounds  in  poetry  seems  to  be  the  only  thing 
one  can  obtain,  and  furthermore,  the  results  obtained  from  in- 
trospecting upon  passages  of  poetry  transmogrified  into  meaning- 
less jargon  justifies  to  a  considerable  degree  the  method  of  intro- 
spection which  most  of  the  subjects  employed.  One  thing  more, 
also;  the  idea  that  the  subjects  would  all  introspect  upon  the 
physical  sensations  or  learn  to  do  so  was  quickly  expelled  from 
the  mind  of  the  experimenter ;  the  threefold  instructions, — to  give 
feeling-tone,  sensations,  and  imagery  proved  to  be  too  autocratic ; 
what  we  have  in  the  above,  typical  introspection  is  mostly  feeling- 
tone  and  association.  However,  inasmuch  as  most  of  the  subjects 
did  not  like  to  think  of  the  movements  of  the  organs  of  articula- 
tion while  they  were  speaking,  to  have  insisted  that  they  do  so 
would  have  been  fatuous. 

All  in  all,  the  introspection  is  the  valuable  part  of  this  set  of 
experiments  so  far  as  we  have  gone  with  them.  Diligent  and 
careful  study  of  the  rank  lists  for  the  above  experiments  has 
failed  to  show  that  changes  in  feeling  tone,  changes  in  the  feel- 
ing with  regard  to  "activity,"  "passivity,"  "energy,"  and  the  like 
states  can  be  traced  down  to  the  numerical  results  with  any  cer- 
tainty. On  the  whole,  however,  the  experiments  in  which  the 
subjects  found  difficulties  of  enunciation  and  the  like,  produced 
the  longest  tappings.  Feelings  of  free  activity  and  pleasurable, 
"dolce  far  niente"  states  usually  correlate  with  lower  tapping 
averages  than  do  other  states.  The  word  "activity,"  however, 
must  not  be  interpreted  to  mean  a  "feeling  of  work"  or  "exer- 
tion" ;  often  it  was  hard  enough  work  to  recite  the  combination, 
but  the  doing  so  may  have  been  pleasant  or  unpleasant,  depending 
upon  many  factors  singly  and  in  a  constellation,  such  as  pleasant 
or  unpleasant  associations,  difficulty  or  ease  of  counting  the  five 
iambics,  and  the  like;  and  inasmuch,  also,  as  we  have  hitherto 
failed  to  attempt  to  "grade"  the  feelings  of  pleasure,  activity,  and 
the  like  numerically,  correlations  of  any  sort  do  not  show  up  with 


44  ROBERT  CHENAULT  GIVLER 

any  degree  of  nicety  either  way.  But  the  grading  of  feeling  tones 
is  a  matter  that  is  on  a  psychological  brink  where  the  footing  is 
horribly  slippery ;  checking  up  the  results  would  have  been  neces- 
sary, and  with  this  kind  of  experiments,  where  the  effective 
phase  appears  to  be  so  fragile  and  at  the  mercy  of  every  other 
psychological  factor,  it  can  hardly  be  supposed,  at  least  from  the 
results  we  have  already  obtained,  that  to  give  these  experimnets 
all  over  again  in  various  orders,  would  have  been  either  wise  or 
fruitful. 

We  next  take  up  the  matter  of  general  correlations  between 
feeling  tone  and  motor  discharge,  taking  all  the  pleasant,  un- 
pleasant, and  neutral  experiments  in  groups  by  themselves  and 
contrasting  the  means,  mean  variations  and  ranges  with  one 
another. 

Subject  PI.  Unp.  'Neut. 

A.  M 95.9  92.7  91.7  PUN 

M.V ....3-0  3-6  3-1  UNP 

Rnj 15  19  17  UNP 

B.  M 88.0         86.1         88.6  N P  U 

M.V 3.1  3.6  4-6  N  P  U 

Rnj 18  17  21  N  P  U 

C.  (as  above) 107.3        IO74        104.8  U  P  N 

2.5  2.6  2.7  N  U  P 

10  n  12  N  U  P 

F.    89.2         89.7        100.1  N  U  P 

3.2  3.1  3.4  N  P  U 

17  18  16  U  P  N 

L 73.8         72.4         74.0  N  P  U 

3-6  3-7  4-1  N  U  P 

16  18  20  N  U  P 

N 86.7         84.8         83.3  PUN 

2.9  2.5  2.7  P  N  U 

IS  ii  14  P  N  U 

T 76.7       73.2  P  N 

34        3-6  N  P 

16  19  N  P 

W 95.1         95.2         95-6  N  U  P 

2.4  2.2  2,5  N  I 

12  10  13  N  P  U 

Y 50.7         52.3         47-1  U  P  N 

3.1  4.1  3-2  UNP 

14  18  17  UNP 


ELEMENTS  OF  SPEECH  IN  RELATION  TO  POETRY         45 

The  difference  between  these  various  averages  is  very  slight, 
and  in  every  case  the  variations  from  it  are  great.  But  they  are 
usually  greater  for  those  which  have  been  obtained  while  the 
subjects  were  in  a  pleasant  state  of  consciousness  than  otherwise. 
Neutrality  and  unpleasantness  appear  to  work  up  to  a  better 
level  in  the  motor  consciousness  than  does  pleasurability ;  fur- 
thermore, introspectively,  there  are  more  varied  states  of  pleas- 
ure than  of  unpleasantness;  if  what  the  subjects  gave  intro- 
spectively is  of  any  importance,  this  appears  to  be  empirically 
substantiated, — they  got  after  a  while  to  be  very  reticent  about  the 
introspecting  upon  the  unpleasant  combinations,  saying  only  such 
things  as  "very  bad,"  "I  do  not  care  for  that  at  all,"  and  the  like. 
But  when  they  got  a  pleasant  combination,  they  would  even  wan- 
der into  forbidden  fields  of  introspection  and  bring  back  material 
which  had  apparently  no  connection  with  the  subject  in  hand.  Not 
all  of  them,  however,  but  it  is  quite  the  fact  that  the  subjects  who 
found  most  of  these  combinations  pleasant,  show  up  the  most 
negatively  in  these  correlations;  at  least  those  who  got  into  the 
most  effervescent  states  of  mind  offer  those  numerical  results 
which  are  the  most  recalcitrant  to  satisfactory  correlation. 

HINTS  OF  A  TONAL  CALCULUS 

We  now  turn  to  the  graphs  for  these  experiments.  Tiren  and 
Tireen  were  taken  together;  and  Unrin  and  Unreen  also;  these 
were  drawn  in  pairs  to  show  the  differences  obtained  from  those 
experiments  which  were  the  nearest  alike.  We  treated  in  the 
same  way  the  graphs  for  Tira  and  Thira,  and  also  for  Rinaz  and 
Rinad.  The  remaining  nineteen  graphs  were  arranged  in  groups 
for  similar  purposes  of  comparison. 

Comparing  Tiren  and  Tireen,  it  appeared  that  the  short  "e"  in 
Tiren  was  responsible  for  the  elevation  of  this  graph  above  the 
other.  Apparently,  also,  the  "long"  E  did  not  "live  up  to  its 
privilege,"  for  the  graphs  were  of  exactly  the  same  length,  even 
group  by  group.  But  the  Tiren  exceeded  the  other  graph  in 
height  only  at  the  beginning  and  end  of  its  course.  Also,  the 
effect  of  the  Tireen  was  steadier  than  that  of  Tiren.  The  latter 
showed  an  average  rise  toward  the  fourth  iambic  and  then  a 


46  ROBERT  CHENAULT  GIVLER 

sudden  descent.  This  also  holds  true  in  all  the  groups  but  the 
last,  as  appeared  from  the  long  Tiren  graph. 

A  comparison  of  the  next  two,  Unrin  and  Unreen,  showed 
less  difference  in  the  general  motor  discharge  aroused  by  these 
two  graphs  than  was  evident  in  the  case  of  the  former  two. 
This  might  be  due  to  a  number  of  things;  first,  the  difference 
in  the  structure  of  the  unaccented  syllables  in  these  pairs:  -un 
may  determine  the  motor  supply  as  much  as  the  -rin  or  the  -reen. 
But  the  differences  in  the  accented  vowels  are  also  to  be  taken 
into  account,  for  in  the  one  pair,  short  "e"  and  long  "e" 
alternated,  while  in  the  other,  short  "i"  alternates  with  long  "e." 
Hence  we  have  two  variables,  and  not  one  to  deal  with. 

Rinaz  and  Thira  produced  the  strongest  effects  of  the  Rinaz- 
Rinad-Thira  group  and  they  were  nearly  equal  in  height  and  very 
similar  in  form;  Rinad  and  Tira  were  exactly  identical  in  form, 
but  not  so  close  together  as  were  the  other  two.  Evidently  "th" 
and  "z"  gave  the  impetus  to  the  responses,  and  the  open  "a"  was 
in  each  case  provocative  of  restraint  in  the  tapping,  for  the  open 
"a"  experiments  took  longer  time  to  utter  than  those  which 
closed  with  "z"  or  "d."  Rinad  was  found  by  the  subjects  to  be  a 
rather  poor  stimulus,  while  they  attributed  to  Rinaz  a  sort  of 
hypnotic  or  lulling  character ;  yet  the  graphs  show  that  the  latter 
of  these  sound-combinations  was  more  arousing  than  the  other. 
But  as  a  general  thing  indifferent  states  were  correlated  Mean- 
wise  with  a  greater  motor  output  than  were  the  pleasant.  But 
Rinaz  was  the  more  pleasant  of  these  two. 

Comparing  Niral  and  Nidal  with  one  another  it  appeared  that 
the  "r"  as  an  initial  accented  consonant  has  a  greater  motor 
effect  than  does  initial  "d."  And  yet  the  "d"  can  be  given  a  much 
more  explosive  vocal  character  than  the  "r."  But  the  long  "i" 
in  Niral  must  not  be  forgotten.  Nemal  showed  very  well,  espe- 
cially in  the  fourth  group,  the  insistent  character  which  was 
attributed  to  it  in  the  introspection.  Comparing  the  lengths  of 
these  graphs  does  not  seem  to  throw  any  light  on  the  matter  of 
correlation,  for  while  the  long  "i"  in  Niral  might  be  construed  as 
that  factor  which  gives  the  length  to  this  graph,  yet  Nerol  is 


ELEMENTS  OF  SPEECH  IN  RELATION  TO  POETRY         47 

equally  long,  but  Nerol  was  the  first  of  this  series  to  be  given, 
and  perhaps  the  subjects  took  longer  to  say  it  because  it  was 
something  new  in  the  way  of  utterance. 

The  next  group  of  graphs  showed  among  other  things,  the* 
various  effects  of  final  "1."  It  is  not  surprising  that  Idrel  took 
a  longer  time  to  repeat  twenty-five  times  than  did  Tirel ;  even  the 
manipulation  of  the  organs  of  articulation  is  a  more  difficult 
matter  for  the  former  combination;  time  is  about  the  only 
noticeable  difference  between  the  two  graphs,  their  height  being 
about  equal.  Undol  and  Unral  were  also  close  quantitative 
equals,  but  the  slightly  greater  effect  of  Unral  at  the  beginning 
of  each  group,  as  was  seen  from  a  combination  graph,  allies  this 
effect  of  "r"  with  those  noted  above.  It  would  seem  then,  that 
sometimes  articulation  force  (e.g.  the  explosive  character  of 
some  consonants)  is  represented  in  the  tapping  in  an  inverse 
proportional. 

Tirel  proceeded  more  evenly  across  the  page  than  did  any  other 
of  this  group  of  graphs,  but  in  the  final  summation  graph,  Rinel 
showed  that  the  average  stress  on  each  of  the  twenty-five  iam- 
bics was  exactly  the  same.  Unfortunately  such  summation 
graphs  were  not  duplicated  to  any  but  the  slightest  degree  in 
the  longer  ones  and  so  their  significance  is  doubtful;  one  point 
is  to  be  made,  however,  and  that  is  that  where  lack  of  uniformity 
between  the  separate  groups  of  the  larger  graphs  is  manifest, 
all  that  can  be  stated  about  the  summation  graphs  as  regards  one 
another  is  just  as  significant  as  that  which  can  be  stated  about 
the  longer  graphs. 

In  regard  to  the  general  effect  of  final  "1"  in  these  combina- 
tions, one  thing  is  quite  remarkable;  and  that  is  the  frequency 
with  which  the  fifth  iambic  of  a  group  ends  with  a  descent  in 
the  curve.  It  is  exactly  fifty  per  cent:  about  ten  per  cent  of  the 
time,  also,  there  is  no  change  from  the  fourth  to  the  fifth  foot. 
The  conclusion  seems  to  be  that  the  tendency  of  final  "1"  is  to 
produce  its  greatest  motor  effect  in  some  other  foot  than  the 
fifth,  when  repeated  in  the  manner  employed  in  the  above 
experiments. 


48  ROBERT  CHENAULT  GIVLER 

The  next  five  graphs  cannot  be  so  strictly  compared  with  one 
another,  but  if  we  consider  those  having  long  vowels,  it  appeared 
that  they  extended  slightly  farther  to  the  right  than  did  the 
others.  The  longest,  "Ritin,"  seems  to  offer  some  sort  of  posi- 
tive correlation  in  regard  to  the  long  "i"  in  the  accented  syllable, 
but  by  comparing  it  with  those  which  we  have  hitherto  considered, 
we  find  that  "length"  of  vowel  is  an  equivocal  expression  in 
reference  to  the  motor  consciousness, 

It  is  proper  now  to  see  whether  we  can  deduce  anything  from 
the  above  twenty-seven  experiments  by  combining  them  in 
various  groups,  for  many  of  them  contain  exactly  the  same  fac- 
tors in  either  the  accented  or  the  unaccented  syllable. 

First:  arranging  these  experiments  in  the  order  of  greatest 
to  least  amount  of  motor  discharge  called  forth  in  the  tapping  we 
get  column  I.  The  figures  represent  425  tapped  strokes  (9  sub- 
jects, each  one  tapping  25  times.)  Opposite  these  numbers  are 
the  feeling  tone  judgments  of  P,  U,  and  N,  with  the  number 
of  times  each  judgment  was  made,  regardless  of  which  subjects 
contributed  to  that  particular  mass  of  judgments. 

PUN 

Idrel    88.4  5           3            i 

Tirel 87.4  8            i            o 

Tulin    87.0  6            i            2 

Nemal    86.8  333 

Rolen  86.8  6           2            i 

Ritin    85.8  324 

Thinal   854  I            7            I 

Niral    85.4  441 

Thira   84.8  432 

Rinaz   84.8  8            I            o 

Af  en 84.6  513 

Undol 84.4  7            n           i 

Unral  844  333 

Rineen  84.0  450 

Unrin 83.6  234 

Nerol  83.6  702 

Tiren 83.6  5           3            i 

Unreen    83.4  441 

Ridev  83.2  252 

Rinel    83.0  522 

Rinad  82.8  333 

Nirees   82.8  720 

Nidal    824  522 

Tireen   82.0  621 

Tira    81.8  5           i            3 

Nilur    81.6  252 

Rileet  .  80.8  8           i           o 


ELEMENTS  OF  SPEECH  IN  RELATION  TO  POETRY         49 

If,  now,  one  arranges  these  same  experiments  in  two  columns, 
one  indicating  a  decline  in  the  amount  of  motor  discharge,  and 
the  other  a  descent  in  pleasurability,  it  can  be  seen  how  far,  gen- 
erally speaking,  these  two  factors  are  correlative.  Likewise, 
taking  the  motor  decline  once  more  and  pairing  it  with  another 
list  indicating  the  decrease  in  unpleasantness  for  the  same  ex- 
periments, another  set  of  correlations  can  be  obtained.  We 
have  determined  the  pleasurability  and  unpleasurability  on  the 
following  basis:  where  under  the  P  column  or  the  U  column 
we  find  the  same  numbers,  e.g.  PUN  and  P  U  N,  we  have 

522  531 

called  the  first  of  these  the  more  pleasant,  and  the  second  the 
more  unpleasant;  where  we  find  the  following  situation 
PUN  P  U  N,  we  have  called  the  second  of  them  the  more 
612  711 

unpleasant.  Here,  as  in  the  former  experiments  upon  the  simple 
vowels  and  consonants,  we  find  that  pleasantness  and  amount 
of  motor  discharge  are  inversely  correlated.  The  average  diver- 
gence of  the  position  of  terms  in  each  column  is  about  ten  points 
away  from  a  one  to  one  correspondence.  We  now  come  to 
another  interesting  phenomenon.  Taking  those  experiments 
which  are  nearest  alike,  and  computing  from  their  difference 
the  effect  of  one  vowel  over  another,  or  one  consonant  over 
another  in  the  motor  consciousness,  we  can  draw  the  following 
tentative  conclusions.  To  begin  with  pairs  of  experiments: 
Exp.  M.D.  PUN  Here  the  unaccented  "Th" 

Thira       84.8  432         has  a  greater  motor  effect  than 

Tira  81.8  5  i  3  does  "T."  Also  the  feeling  tone 
correlation  is  strong  for  the  increased  motor  effect  of  the  un- 
pleasant. Take  another: 

Exp.        M.D.         PUN  Here  short  "i"  accented  has 

Unrin     83.6          234        a  greater  motor  effect  than  does 

Unreen    83.4  441         long  "i";  and  the  same  feeling 

tone  correlation  also  holds  true  as  with  the  above. 

Again,  Exp.        M.D.         PUN  In  this  pair  the  short 

Tiren       83.6  531         vowel   may   be   credited 

Tireen     82.0  621         with  the  greater  motor 

effect. 


ROBERT  CHENAULT  GIVLER 


And  in  Exp.  M.D. 
Rinaz  84.8 
Rinad  82.8 

greater  motor  effect. 

inferences  are  dubious. 


PUN  We  find  the  accented 

8     i     o        find    "z"    as    that    ele- 

333         ment   which   gives   the 

But  in  both  of  the  above,  feeling  tone 

Let  us  now  compare  several  groups : 

Here  the  long  accented 
vowels  seem  to  have  the  ad- 
vantage, and  the  "naz"  and 
"nad"  do  not  contradict  the 
deductions  about  them  made 
immediately  before. 

Here  the  short  vowels  again 
produce,  or  assist  in  produc- 
ing, the  greater  motor  effect. 
Deductions  on  the  basis  of 
feeling  tone  are  hardly  possi- 
groups.  We  .make  two  more 


Here  the  feeling  tone  en- 
ters   again    as    an    apparent 
factor,    but    rather   contradic- 
torily  to   what   has  been  de- 
duced from  the  experiments  as  a  whole. 

Exp.  M.D.  PUN 

Un-ral  84.4  333 

Un-dol  84.4  711  Which  completes  our  groups 

Un-rin  83.6  234         of  this  kind. 

Un-reen  83.4  441 

In  connection  with  the  next  matter,  a  few  symbols  are  neces- 
sary. Let  Ki  =  the  accented  consonant  which  stands  imme- 
diately before  the  accented  vowel,  thus:  ni-Ral,  un-Dol,  etc. 
Let  also  K2  =  that  accented  consonant  standing  immediately 
after  the  accented  vowel,  or  which  closes  the  accented  syllable, 


Exp. 

M.D. 

P 

U    N 

Ri-tin 

85.8 

3 

2     4 

Ri-naz 

84.8 

8 

I       O 

Ri-neen 

84.0 

4 

5    o 

Ri-dev 

83.2 

2 

5    2 

Ri-nel 

83.0 

5 

2      2 

Ri-nad 

82.8 

3 

3     3 

Ri-leet 

80.8 

8 

I       O 

Exp. 

M.D. 

P 

U    N 

Ti-rel 

87.4 

8 

I       0 

Ti-ren 

83-6 

5 

3     i 

Ti-reen 

82.0 

4 

4     i 

Ti-ra 

81.8 

5 

i     3 

ble    in 

either    of 

these    two 

comparisons : 


Exp. 

M.D. 

PUN 

Ni-rees 

82.8 

720 

Ni-dal 

82.4 

522 

Ni-lur 

81.6 

2       C      2 

ELEMENTS  OF  SPEECH  IN  RELATION  TO  POETRY         51 

thus:  ni-raL,  ri-niZ,  etc.  Let  also  V  stand  for  the  accented 
vowel,  and  let  the  letters  i,  e,  a,  etc.,  represent  the  short  vowels, 
and  the  letters  I,  E,  A,  etc.,  represent  the  long  vowels.  Let 
also  the  symbol  >  stand  for  the  "greater  than"  and  the  symbol 
<  stand  for  "less  than."  Then,  in  general,  taking  all  the  com- 
monly used  accented  final  and  initial  consonants  of  the  accented 
syllables,  we  have : 

>  K2N  =  17.4  >    15.5 

>  VE    =  17.3  >   12.0 

>  VA    =  17.5  >   12.8 

and  KiL  >  KiR  >  KiN  =  17.0  >   14.0  >   13.6, 

so  that  the  motor  effect  of  KiN  <  the  motor  effect  of  KiR  and 
also  <  that  of  KiL.  We  can  say,  then,  that  of  the  consonants, 
KiL  and  K2L  have  the  greatest  motor  effect,  and  the  short 
vowels  also,  in  general,  rank  with  them.  This  is  significant,  and 
shall  be  used  later  in  the  correlation  of  large  masses  of  sounds. 

3.     TRANSMOGRIFICATIONS   OF   ENGLISH   POETRY 

This  part  of  the  work  includes  several  kinds  of  experiments. 
According  to  the  plan  outlined  in  the  early  paragraphs,  we  are 
now  to  consider  the  effect  of  more  complicated  collocations  of 
the  speech  elements  than  have  been  hitherto  employed.  The  poets 
themselves  are  experimented  upon  from  now  on,  and  in  a  two- 
fold manner :  first,  by  casting  into  decasyllabic  lines  the  sounds 
as  they  appear  in  the  tabulations  of  frequency,  and  then  by  em- 
ploying both  single  lines  and  ten-line  passages  from  poetry  to 
show  the  various  effects  of  combination  with  and  without 
meaning. 

THE  EFFECT  OF  MEANINGLESS  SINGLE  LINES  FROM  THE  TABLES 
OF  SOUND  FREQUENCY 

We  now  turn  to  the  first  of  these  experiments.  They  were 
devised  to  show  the  psycho-motor  effect  of  those  sounds  which 
certain  poets  use  most  frequently  as  well  as  those  which  they 
use  less  frequently,  and  in  each  case  they  attempted  to  illustrate 
the  proportion  of  use  by  the  frequency  of  repetition  in  the 


52  ROBERT  CHENAULT  GIVLER 

decasyllabic  line  into  which  they  were  arranged.  Taking  four 
of  the  poets  from  the  tables  of  sound  frequency  hitherto  men- 
tioned, we  find  them  using  the  sounds  of  the  language  in  the 
following  proportions.  We  take  only  the  first  twenty  or  more 
sounds  from  each  one,  accented  and  unaccented  alike.  (Short 
vowels,  small  letters;  long  ones,  large.) 


Keats 

Byron 

Tennyson 

Arnold 

Ace. 

Unacc.    Ace. 

Unacc.      Ace. 

Unacc.    Ace. 

Unacc. 

R 

3-9 

U 

5-3  :R 

5-3 

u 

5.3  :R 

5-0 

u 

5-3  :R 

5-5  u 

4.0 

N 

3-7 

i 

4.1  :N 

4.0 

N 

4-5  :L 

4-5 

i 

4.0  :N 

4.6  i 

34 

L 

3-5 

N 

3-5  :L 

3-5 

i 

34  :T 

3-1 

N 

3-2  :L 

3-6  N 

3-0 

S 

34 

R 

3Ji  :E 

2.19 

R 

2.7  :M 

2.7: 

:Th 

2.9  :T 

3-3  a 

2t9 

T 

3-1 

T 

2.3  :D 

2-5 

T 

3-1  :S 

2.6 

R 

2.8  :S 

3-1  D 

2.6 

D 

2-5' 

S 

2.0  :M 

2.4 

Th 

2.3  :E 

2-5 

D 

2.1  :E 

2.3  Th 

2.5 

e 

2.1 

Th 

1.9:8 

24 

€ 

1.7  :a 

2.3 

a 

1.8:  A 

2.2  L 

2.1 

V 

2.O 

e 

1.7  :T 

2.2 

Z 

i.7:I 

2.1 

L 

1.8  :D 

2.2  Z 

1.8 

u 

1.9 

O 

11.7  :0 

2.1 

O 

1.6  :D 

2.O 

Z 

1.8  :0 

2.1    R 

1-7 

E 

1.8 

L 

1.6:  A 

1.8 

D 

i>.6  :N 

2.O 

T 

1.8  :F 

2.1    E 

1-7 

P 

17 

a 

1.5  :u 

1.8 

a 

US  :Z 

11.9 

Th* 

i.6:I 

2.0  T 

1.6 

M 

1-9 

D 

i.5:I 

1.6 

I 

1.4  :P 

1.9 

Ng 

i.o:M 

1.8  e 

14 

A 

i-7 

K 

1.4  :e 

1-5 

E 

14:1 

1.8 

W 

i.  0:0 

1.5  H 

14 

F 

1-7 

Ng 

i.i  :a 

1.4 

Th* 

1.3  :B 

1-7 

E 

.9:6 

14  Th* 

1-3 

i 

1.6 

Th* 

i.i  :i 

14 

o 

1.2:0 

1.6 

I 

.9:K 

14  W 

i-3 

K 

1.6 

M 

i.o:P 

14 

L 

1.2  :F 

1.4 

e 

.8:W 

i.3  S 

i.i 

H 

1-4 

W 

1.0  :K 

1.2 

S 

1.0  :W 

1.4 

e 

.7:Z 

1.3  B 

1.0 

I 

1.3 

E 

.9:0 

I.I 

M 

.9:u 

1-3 

0 

.7:1 

13  A 

•9 

0 

1.3 

F 

.8:F 

I.I 

W 

.8:K 

1-3 

H 

.7:u 

1.2   K 

.7 

w 

1.2 

V 

.8:V 

1-3 

B 

.8:e 

1.2 

OO 

.6:a 

i.o  Ng 

•7 

z 

1.2 

Z 

.7:Th 

I.I 

P 

.8:0 

1.2 

K 

.6:B 

I.O 

< 

:G 

1.2 

St 

.6:P 

I.O 

:St 

I.O 

*  Surd. 

All  but  one  of  these  poets,  Byron,  employs  his  sounds  ap- 
proximately in  the  ratio  of  ten  accented  to  eight  unaccented  ones. 
Byron  uses  nine  unaccented  ones  to  every  ten  accented  ones.  We 
took  the  first  seven  accented  consonants  and  the  first  three  accented 
vowels,  and  the  first  five  unaccented  consonants  and  the  first  three 
unaccented  vowels  from  the  other  three  poets;  in  Byron's  case 
the  same  number  of  accented  sounds  were  used,  but  six  unaccented 
consonants  and  three  unaccented  vowels  were  taken  to  make  his 
10:9  proportion.  Taking  then,  ten  accented  sounds  from  each 
of  these  poets,  and  the  proportionate  number  of  unaccented 
sounds,  and  arranging  them  in  a  line  of  five  iambics  with  the 


ELEMENTS  OF  SPEECH  IN  RELATION  TO  POETRY         53 

most  used  sounds  in  the  more  prominent  places  in  the  line, 
namely  the  first  and  the  last  feet,  we  obtain  the  following  four 
experiments : 

Keats:     Ni  rul  su  veed  ri  nest  it  el  ith  reen. 
Byron :    Zu  reen  tith  nole  der  tull  thin  sode  tu  reem. 
Tennyson :    Thu  ral  di  reet  thu  nime  ra  dees  un  tal. 
Arnold :    Thu  reen  din  lase  za  f ode  ml  tane  thu  leer. 

The  10:8  and  the  10:9  proportions  are  fairly  well  kept  in 
these  combinations,  as  can  be  proven  by  counting  the  sounds. 
The  arrangement  of  sounds  is  quite  arbitrary,  but  in  making 
such  a  combination,  there  are  many  things  to  be  considered  be- 
sides mere  proportion.  For  example,  one  must  test  by  means  of 
his  ear  what  combinations  are  suitable  to  follow  one  another; 
and  when  strict  proportion  cannot  be  followed,  one  must  be 
judicious. 

If,  now,  we  take  the  next  ten  accented  sounds  in  the  lists,  and 
combine  with  them  the  proper  number  of  the  next  eight  or  nine 
unaccented  sounds,  we  have  the  following  experiments  for  these 
poets: 

Keats :    La  maz  mo  kif  de  poth  la  pam  we  ho. 
Byron :    Do  thav  16  pi  se  ke  da  thip  H  va. 
Tennyson :    Te  poz  li  bin  eng  wo  zite  nuf  we  nop. 
Arnold :    Re  moz  et  wik  sa  thu  we  zem  he  mi. 

(It  must  be  noticed  that  when  the  appearance  of  the  list  indicates 
that  more  than  three  vowels  to  seven  consonants  are  the  materials 
out  of  which  the  experiment  would  normally  be  made,  we  have 
followed  the  list,  rather  than  discarded  the  prominent  elements 
in  it.) 

These  experiments  were  presented  in  pairs:  first  the  two 
representing  Keats,  and  after  that  the  others  in  the  order  in 
which  they  occur  above,  finishing  each  poet  as  we  proceeded. 
Each  line  was  tapped  and  recited  five  times.  The  instructions 
were  to  read  the  line  over  until  it  became  easy  to  read;  but  not 
to  wait  until  associations  came  up;  then  the  tapping  and  reciting 
went  on  as  it  had  done  before,  the  instructions  again  being  to 


54  ROBERT  CHENAULT  GIVLER 

"tap  at  each  accented  syllable."  A  few  of  the  subjects  prac- 
tised reading  the  lines  over  once  silently  and  tapping  at  the  same 
time,  in  order  that  full  preparation  for  the  experiment  would 
not  be  lacking.  The  idea  was  to  get  the  full  effect  of  the  line 
while  tapping,  in  order  that  the  motor  manifestations  could 
be  called  representative  in  the  fullest  sense  of  the  word.  The 
introspection  which  they  gave  for  these  experiments  follows : 

Keats  I.  (The  first  ten  accented  and  the  first  eight  unac- 
cented sounds.) 

Visual  imagery  of  maples  and  other  deciduous  trees  in  the  fall  of  the  year; 
the  air  is  frosty  and  the  whole  soene  is  grayish;  very  objective. 

The  rhythm  feels  like  dance  music;  very  contagious;  thought  of  evening 
bells ;  very  romantic.  Gentle  thing ;  not  much  action  to  it ;  a  little  Spencerian ; 
deals  with  pastoral  things. 

Keats  II.     (The  second  group  of  Keats'  most  used  sounds.) 
Visual  and  auditory  imagery  of  the  surf;  also  of  fields;  sounds  go  well 

together.     Seems  short  and  broken  up;  compares  it  to  an  "Italian"  salad. 

Brings  imagery  of  the  sea;  thinks  of  something  like  the  "Ancient  Mariner." 

Not  very  active. 

Byron  I. 

Something  superficial  about  it;  kinaesthetic  and  visual  imagery  of  idly 
following  things  about.  Minor,  meditative  and  solemn;  attributes  this  to 
the  long  vowels;  thinks  of  something  like  "Crossing  the  Bar."  An  epic, 
recounting  adventures.  Deep  and  funereal. 

Byron  II. 

Active  thing;  relates  to  strength  and  power  and  brawny  arms.  Sounded 
like  the  speech  of  a  big-chested,  half -civilized  people  living  in  a  cold  climate. 
Rather  dramatic  and  superficial. 

Tennyson  I. 

Inactive;  sad  throughout;  subjective  thing.  Evokes  pity  and  compassion; 
elegiac  thing;  sorrow,  not  wild,  but  quiet  and  domestic  implied.  Very 
rhythmic ;  gives  a  feeling  of  solemnity  felt  in  the  midst  of  happy  surroundings. 
Images  of  a  quiet,  restful  woodland  scene.  Asked  if  it  was  Tennyson.  Just  a 
little  dramatic;  not  quite  sincere.  A  little  tragic  and  sorrowful;  thinks  of 
Fate. 

Tennyson  II. 

Rhythmical  and  dramatic;  sounds  Shakesperean.  Kinaesthetic  imagery  of 
many  motions  in  different  directions.  Makes  him  short  of  breath;  thinks  of 
dying  gladiators ;  mentioned  "Heldenleben."  An  explanation  in  some  dialect 
of  an  unfortunate  event. 


ELEMENTS  OF  SPEECH  IN  RELATION  TO  POETRY         55 

Arnold  I. 

Something  strong  and  military  about  it.  Rhythmic,  but  not  very  deep. 
Medieval  and  romantic;  seems  to  be  a  description.  Thinks  of  the  "Meister- 
singers";  active  and  objective. 

Arnold  II. 

Unpleasant  taste  images  aroused;  something  psycho-pathic  about  it.  Inter- 
esting, but  not  emotional;  full  of  irritating  things;  more  like  conversation 
than  anything  else.  Leaves  one  a  little  gasping  ;  sounds  like  some  one  timidly 
trying  to  recite. 

It  must  be  remembered  that  these  combinations  were  pre- 
sented to  the  subjects  without  their  knowing  what  poets  they 
represented;  nor  was  it  a  guessing  contest,  either;  no  stress  was 
laid  upon  anything  but  a  good  reading  of  the  lines. 

There  follows  the  correlation  between  feeling  tone  and  motor 
discharge,  in  general,  for  all  the  subjects  in  the  above  experiments. 

Subject 

A.  M  .......................  94.3  91.0  104.9  NPU 

M.V  ......................     2.1  2.0  3.4  NPU 

Rnj  ......................   10  12  16  N  U  P 

B.  (as  above)  ...............  74.0         -          72.2  P  N 

34  3-6  N  P 

15  17  N  P 

C.  "        "     ..............  92.5         -         82.0  P  N 

3.6         -  5-5  N  P 

19  27  N  P 


F.         "       "    ..............  73-5  75-7  -  UP 

3.6  3-8  -  U  P 

17  18  UP 

L.         "       "     ..............  72.0  74.8  -  U  P 

3-2  2.7  -  P  U 

18  15  P  U 

N.         "        "     ..............  85.2  84.6  -  P  U 

2.4  2.O  -  P   U 

12  II  P   U 

T.        "       "     ..............  75-3  774  -  UP 

3-0  3-7  -  U  P 

20  13  -  P  U 

W.       "        "     ..............  87.8  85.9  91.2  NPU 

2.3  2.5  24  U  N  P 

10  13  12  U  N  P 

Y.        "       "    ..............  39.9  41.9  52.0  N  U  P 

2.3  3-2  3-0  U  N  P 

IS  17  19  N  U  P 


56  ROBERT  CHENAULT  GIVLER 

Comparing  these  results  with  all  similar  correlations,  we 
find  that  none  of  the  subjects  have  remained  constant  during 
the  experiments,  as  is  evidenced  by  the  following.  A.'s  longest 
tappings  were  twice  for  the  pleasant,  twice  for  the  unpleasant  and 
twice  for  the  neutral.  B.'s  were  three  times  for  the  pleasant, 
twice  for  the  unpleasant,  and  once  for  the  neutral,  etc.  The 
whole  list  is  given  below. 


(this  subject  did  not  begin  with  the  others) 

(only  one  kind  of  judgment  was  made  in  one  series) 

(only  one  kind  of  judgment  was  made  in  one  series) 
(this  subject  did  not  begin  with  the  others,  and  in 

one  series  made  only  one  kind  of  judgments) 
19         14 

Comparing  the  M.V.  and  the  Rnj.  in  a  similar  manner  gives 
equally  varied  results. 

Let  us  now  consider  the  graphs  for  these  experiments.  Their 
resemblances  were  more  striking  than  their  differences,  and  it  is 
not  unlikely  that  the  reverberations  set  up  in  the  motor  con- 
sciousness just  by  this  new  material  were  too  strong  to  be  altered 
by  the  other  factors  involved.  In  the  first  place  it  was  found 
that  these  experiments  produced  a  much  lower  motor  output  than 
did  those  preceding  them ;  Idrel  had  reached  a  height  of  88.4  mm. ; 
none  of  these  reach  anything  above  78.8  mm.  (the  Arnold  II). 
Massing  together  the  P,  U,  and  N  judgments,  and  correlating 
them  with  the  amounts  of  motor  discharge  as  shown  by  these 
graphs,  we  obtain: 

PUN 

Arnold  II M.D.  av.  78.8  mm.  342 

Tennyson  1 78.6  711 

Byron  II 78.4  8           o            r 

Tennyson  II 78.0  441 

Arnold  1 77.6  4           4            i 

Byron  1 77.4  540 

Keats  II 77.2  711 

Keats  1 76.6  711 

This  comparison  would  seem  to  indicate  that  the  most  pleasant 
experiments  go  with  the  strongest  and  the  weakest  tappings, 


ELEMENTS  OF  SPEECH  IN  RELATION  TO  POETRY         57 

while  the  most  unpleasant  tend  to  produce  those  which  are  mid- 
way between  the  greatest  and  the  least.  No  correlation  with  the 
feelings  of  activity  adds  anything  to  what  we  have  already  ob- 
served, that  sometimes,  and  sometimes  only,  the  feeling  of  ease 
means  longer  finger  strokes,  and  feelings  of  inhibition  and  diffi- 
culty mean  shorter  strokes. 

None  of  the  experiments  numbered  III  are  given  for  any  of 
the  poets,  and  hereafter  only  No.  I  of  the  first  three  is  presented, 
because  it  represents  better  than  do  the  others  the  differences 
in  sound  frequency  for  the  poet.  No.  Ill,  however,  was  con- 
structed by  combining  half  of  the  first  and  half  of  the  second 
of  the  above  groups  of  sounds,  Nos.  I  and  II,  to  illustrate  the 
poet's  use  of  those  elements  which  are  not  either  very  frequent 
nor  infrequent,  and  thus  we  had  a  set  of  three  experiments  which 
began  with  the  liquids,  and  ended  with  the  gutterals  and 
fricatives. 

Experiments  numbered  IV,  V,  and  VI  in  the  case  of  each 
poet  are  transmogrifications  of  single  lines  of  poetry;  experi- 
ments VII,  VIII,  and  IX  are  lines  of  poetry  rearranged, 
keeping  the  accented  words  of  the  original  accented,  and  the 
unaccented  words  unaccented.  Experiments  X,  XI,  and  XII  are 
lines  of  poetry,  "clothed  and  in  their  right  mind,"  and  for 
each  of  the  twenty  poets  experimented  upon  there  are  these 
twelve  experiments,  which  start  with  the  tonal  elements,  pass 
through  the  nonsense  verse  of  transmogrification,  through  also 
the  ungrammatical  poetry  into  the  normal  meaningful  lines 
from  which  all  had  been  ultimately  derived  that  preceded  them. 
Thus  we  had  240  experiments  upon  the  poets  arranged  in  such 
a  way  as  to  supply  sufficient  data  for  a  compact  thesis  in  itself. 
In  the  case  of  nine  of  the  twenty  poets,  the  same  material  en- 
tered into  the  transmogrifications,  rearrangements  and  the  mean- 
ingful lines,  so  that  Experiments  IV,  V,  and  VI  were  the  trans- 
mogrifications of  the  material  in  Experiments  X,  XI  and  XII 
respectively,  and  Experiments  VII,  VIII  and  IX  were  the  re- 
arrangements of  the  same  material  as  had  been  used  in  the  others. 
In  the  other  eleven  poets,  which  were  experimented  upon  first, 


58  ROBERT  CHENAULT  GIVLER 

only  now  and  then  was  such  a  symmetrical  arrangement  carried 
out.  We  decided  upon  the  method  of  repeating  the  same  sounds 
in  three  different  relations  in  order  to  exactly  determine  what 
both  grammar  and  meaning  had  to  do  with  the  effects  of  the 
poetic  line  upon  the  motor  and  the  introspective  consciousness. 
But  only  in  the  case  of  four  of  the  poets  do  we  give  the  experi- 
ments from  IV  to  XII.  What  has  been  omitted  will  be  con- 
sidered in  later,  summarizing  paragraphs. 

It  was  the  purpose  at  first  to  obtain  experimental  material 
from  the  poets  which  would  duplicate  in  tonal  quality  Experi- 
ments I,  II,  and  III;  after  a  futile  search  for  such  lines,  it  was 
given  up;  too  many  extraneous  elements  entered  into  the  matter. 
In  the  first  place,  very  few  lines  of  the  poet  actually  follow  the 
tonal  pattern  of  the  first  experiment  in  our  series,  and  those 
that  do,  usually  contain  inverted  iambics,  if  they  contain  iambics 
at  all ;  in  the  second  place,  to  find  decasyllabic  lines  that  are  regu- 
larly accented  in  the  iambic  pattern  is  not  always  possible,  and 
when  such  are  found,  they  are  likely  to  be  some  of  the  poorest 
lines,  esthetically  considered,  that  the  poet  suffered  to  leave  his 
pen.  So  that  in  every  case  almost,  Experiments  I,  II,  and  III 
are  in  a  class  by  themselves,  and  the  other  nine  experiments  of  the 
series  are  in  another  class. 

THE  TRANSMOGRIFICATION  OF  SINGLE  LINES  OF  POETRY 

The  business  of  transmogrifying  single  lines  of  poetry  is 
quite  difficult,  for  the  restrictions  placed  upon  one  in  this  work 
are  very  rigid, — words  must  not  be  made,  and  sounds  must  not 
be  left  out;  again,  to  avoid  making  words,  one  may  have  to 
construct  a  very  badly  sounding  line,  and  sacrifice  to  the  purpose 
of  the  experiment  much  of  his  artistic  predilection  for  the  beauty 
of  tones;  let  any  one  who  doubts  the  difficulty  of  the  matter  at- 
tempt the  task,  and  he  will  find  that  we  have  but  lightly  touched 
on  the  obstacles  to  be  encountered.  The  transmogrifications  we 
have  used  in  this  experiment  are  but  a  fifth  of  the  number  which 
we  attempted  to  complete  with  satisfaction  to  ourselves. 

These  experiments  were  presented  in  the  order  in  which  they 
are  given  in  the  following  pages.  The  first  four  poets  experi- 


ELEMENTS  OF  SPEECH  IN  RELATION  TO  POETRY          59 

mented  upon  were  Byron,  Keats,  Arnold  and  Tennyson,  and 
the  forty-eight  experiments  upon  them  were  well  under  way  be- 
fore the  I  and  E  Experiments  had  been  completed.  This  was  a 
relief  to  the  subjects,  who  rather  chafed  at  the  idea  of  having 
so  many  similar  combinations  week  after  week. 

There  were  fifteen  experiments  performed  on  Keats,  but  we 
have  omitted  all  but  twelve  in  our  consideration,  because  we 
discovered  that  the  others  were  of  no  importance  for  the  work 
in  hand.  Our  original  plan  had  been  to  experiment  upon  very 
many  more  than  twelve  decasyllabic  lines  for  each  poet,  but 
time  did  not  allow,  and  this  had  to  be  given  up.  Neither  were 
the  subjects  able  to  react  to  twenty  experiments  an  hour  as  well  as 
to  twelve. 

The  introspection  for  the  Keats  and  Byron  experiments  is 
very  interesting  indeed,  from  the  very  start,  and  the  transmogri- 
fication of  the  famous  line  from  Byron's  "Apostrophe  to  the 
Ocean"  was  a  decided  success,  but  it  is  not  beyond  cavil  that 
the  line  may  have  contained  too  many  hints  at  the  sense  of  the 
original  verse. 

After  the  introspective  account  of  the  matter,  we  have  given 
the  numerical  results  which  we  shall  consider  at  once. 

Keats  IV.  Won  femz  e  nanj  6v  di  nal  ter  en  teev.,*  trans- 
mogrified from  "One  faint,  eternal  eventide  of  gems." 

Nothing  in  the  introspection  compared  to  the  original  mean- 
ing or  mood  of  the  line. 

Keats  V.  Too  zowb  ith  bind  tha  rawl  me  ting  win  ti.  From 
"To  bind  them  all  about  with  tiny  rings."  Introspection. 

Visual  imagery  of  a  farm;  auditory  imagery  of  some  bells  ringing.  Sounds 
like  an  ode;  vocal  placement  seemed  forward  in  the  mouth.  Gives  a  light 
and  cool  effect;  the  sounds  vary  in  pitch  very  much;  more  resonant  than 
the  former  one. 

*  From  now  on  the  diacritical  marks  will  have  their  usual  significance  over 
single  vowels ;  but  when  two  vowels  occur  together,  the  following  interpre- 
tation is  required :  ee  =  e ;  oo  =  the  oo  in  "brook" ;  66  =  the  oo  in  "food", 
etc;  a  as  in  "ask"  is  indicated  by  no  mark  at  all;  in  addition  to  this,  the 
ow,  aw,  ou,  oi,  and  other  diphthongal  sounds  are  pronounced  as  usual  in 
'English.  Differences  between  the  sonant  and  surd  th,  are  not  indicated  here,, 
though  they  were  in  the  experiment. 


60  ROBERT  CHENAULT  GIVLER 

Keats  VI.  A  thurn  us  too  thu  bow  too  flandry  bide.  From. 
"A  flow'ry  band  to  bind  us  to  the  earth." 

Very  rhythmic  line;  'Chaucer's  poetry  came  to  mind.  Imagery  of  a  high 
tower;  romantic  scene,  maybe  some  battle  being  narrated;  romantic  and 
idyllic.  "Flaundry"  a  strange  word;  thinks  of  "Flanders"  and  "laundry" 
at  the  same  time. 

Keats.  Experiment  VII.  So  cooling  very  still  was  and  the 
air.  From  "The  air  was  cooling  and  so  very  still." 

Imagery  of  the  twilight.  Visual  imagery  of  a  bright  green  color.  Cool 
sensation  in  the  mouth.  Very  conscious  of  lips. 

Keats  VIII.  The  dwindled  of  its  trace  and  edgings  brim. 
From  "The  trace  and  dwindled  edgings  of  its  brim." 

Imagery  of  snow;  "trace"  a  "cold"  word.    Feels  lips  to  be  very  active. 

Keats  IX.  Across  the  move  would  blue  a  little  cloud.  From 
"A  little  cloud  would  move  across  the  blue." 

Gives  an  inane  feeling;  "move"  and  "blue"  are  "sticky"  words;  "cloud" 
doesn't  go  with  them.  Cannot  say  "little  cloud"  fast  enough  to  suit  the  line. 
The  word  "across"  is  too  hard ;  the  "k"  sound  sticks.  Lips  rather  prominent 
in  consciousness. 

Keats  X.    A  bower  quiet  for  us  and  a  sleep. 

Odd  that  the  gutteral  should  have  been  used, — "quiet."  "Bower"  the  only 
"quiet"  sound.  Uses  too  much  breath  for  the  meaning. 

Keats  XI.    "With  lucent  syrops  tinct  with  cinnamon." 

Thinks  of  toddy  and  cordials;  just  the  opposite  kind  of  a  scene  came  up. 
Much  taste  imagery;  visual  imagery  in  bright  colors.  The  line  hisses  too 
much;  imagery  of  peppermint.  Tickles  the  tongue. 

Keats  XII.    "That  broodest  o'er  the  troubled  sea  of  mind." 

Mind  in  a  quiet  uncertainty.  Thought  of  Byron,  and  Hamlet;  visualized  a 
cliff.  Feeling  of  a  cosmic  melancholy.  Gives  a  gentle  melancholy;  "mind" 
too  abrupt. 

Byron  IV.  Shun  dole  ow  rod  thu  nark  blore  6  land  eep.  From 
"Roll  on  thou  dark  and  deep  blue  ocean,  roll." 

Visual  imagery  of  some  one  on  a  rock  by  the  sea ;  sounded  like  a  foreign 
language.  Subject  A. — Imagery  of  the  ocean,  in  a  storm;  the  jerky  effect 
here  is  justifiable.  B. — Imagery  of  a  ship  on  the  ocean  in  stormy  weather; 
seemed  to  take  more  force.  D. — Imagery  of  the  sea;  heavy  waves;  dark 
colors.  F. — Sea  imagery;  "nark"  is  very  hard.  "Blore  o"  is  the  blowing  of 
a  horn.  K. — 'Great  deal  of  resonance;  no  imagery.  L. — Sea  concepts  aroused; 
thinks  of  Norsemen,  etc.;  very  thrilling.  M. — Feels  the  roar  of  the  ocean; 
visual  imagery  very  rich.  P. — Counting  bothered  a  little;  imagery  of  Hoi- 


ELEMENTS  OF  SPEECH  IN  RELATION  TO  POETRY         61 

land.     S. — Mixture  of  Persian,  Arabic  and  Hindustani  language;  great  deal 
of  imagery  of  the  ocean;  sounds  fill  the  mouth. 

Byron  V.  Thu  vl  land  sa  ml  ji  ta  fro  the  vee.  From  "The 
inviolate  island  of  the  sage  and  free"  (with  two  unaccented 
syllables  omitted). 

Narrative  poetry ;  thinks  of  natural  scenes.  A  gesture  could  do  it  all  better 
than  words.  Imagery  of  gaudy  colors ;  not  smooth  sounds.  Not  very  smooth ; 
especially  the  fourth  iambic;  imagery  of  a  pastoral  scene.  Imagery  of  some 
big  man  saying  this  in  a  thunderous  voice.  Lip  sensations  unpleasant. 

Byron  VI.  Has  mowd  sti  rme  wha  stesh  ra  mid  thu  krathe. 
From  "Amidst  this  wreck  where  thou  hast  made  a  shrine." 

Describing  a  very  interesting  place;  something  troubled  and  dolorous  about 
it.  Implies  a  rough  activity.  All  the  sounds  very  unpleasant;  do  not  fuse. 
Too  staccato;  but  "su  krath"  very  good.  Just  a  strange,  incomprehensible 
language. 

Byron  VII.  "With  stirred  as  rose  her  dream  leaves  with  the 
air."  From  "Stirred  with  her  dream  as  rose  leaves  with  the 
air." 

Ethereal  quality  about  the  sounds.  Rhythmic;  thought  of  a  sleeping  girl. 
The  words  "air"  and  "stirred'  the  best  of  all;  olfactory  imagery. 

Byron  VIII.  At  bluelit  moon  and  midnight  on  the  deep.  From 
"At  midnight  on  the  blue  and  moonlit  deep." 

Imagery  of  a  ship  at  midnight;  pleasant  self-feelings.  Smooth,  soft,  and 
gentle  combination.  , 

Byron  IX.  Upon  beheld  decline  who  hath  my  brow.  From 
"Who  hath  beheld  decline  upon  my  brow." 

Thinks  of  a  pessimistic  old  man.  Monotonous;  takes  too  much  breath. 
Thinks  of  some  unsuccessful  person. 

Byron  X.    "And  temple  more  divinely  desolate." 

"Emp"  sounds  pointed.  "Desolate"  just  suits  the  meaning;  it's  a  cold, 
blue  word;  "divinely"  doesn't  have  anything  to  do  with  the  line.  The 
"hiss"  in  "desolate"  very  expressive. 

Byron  XI.    "Yet  I  was  born  where  men  are  proud  to  be." 

Prosaic  and  pessimistic;  sounds  very  resonant.  Very  strong  physical 
sensations.  "Yet"  always  a  vocal  surprise. 

Byron  XII.    "And  silent  rows  the  songless  gondolier." 

Very  dark  and  somber  feeling  induced.  "Silent"  the  only  silent  word  in 
the  line;  "gon"  in  "gondolier"  is  too  gutteral.  Cheerful  and  lively  line 
in  spite  of  the  intended  meaning;  "songless  gondolier"  a  tonal  equivocation. 


62  ROBERT  CHENAULT  GIVLER 

The  remarkable  thing  about  the  graphs  representing  these 
twelve  experiments  upon  Byron  and  Keats  was  that  in  nearly 
every  case  the  motor  discharge  for  the  Byron  Experiments  is 
greater  than  that  for  Keats,  and  the  final  averages  showed  that 
the  combined  finger  movements  for  the  Byron  experiments  were 
eighteen  metres  longer  than  they  were  for  Keats.  But  we  must 
not  be  too  sanguine;  the  tonal  elements  in  Byron  are  not  the 
ones  which  have  hitherto  been  those  arousing  the  strongest  re- 
actions, and  the  fact  that  Keats  was  experimented  upon  first  may 
indicate  that  we  have  only  practise  curves  before  us  in  this  in- 
stance. But  there  was  something  rousing  and  enthusing  in  all 
the  Byron  experiments  which  the  subjects  did  not  find  in  those 
on  Keats,  which  may  account  for  the  matter  more  exactly.  The 
character  of  the  lines  transmogrified  and  employed  in  other  ways 
is  very  different  for  the  two  poets,  as  any  one  can  see  from  a 
glance  at  the  material;  but  whatever  may  be  the  nature  of  the 
individual  sounds  used  in  the  above  experiments,  we  find  here 
that  order  and  arrangement  are  potent  factors,  and  single  deca- 
syllabic lines  may  produce  different  effects  than  do  larger  pas- 
sages. One  could  also  notice  in  these  graphs  the  form  quality 
of  the  decasyllabic  groups, — the  regular  thing  in  a  rhythmic  line 
is  for  the  first  and  the  last  feet  to  demand  more  motor  discharge 
than  the  intermediate  iambics.  And  the  form-quality  of  the 
Byron  lines  always  differed  from  those  of  Keats.  Again,  the 
mean  variation  of  the  tappings  for  the  meaningful  lines  is  less, 
according  to  the  graphs,  than  it  is  in  the  first  six  of  the  experi- 
ments for  these  poets. 

Every  one  will  admit  that  Byron  is  a  more  oratorical  poet  than 
Keats,  and  that  there  is  a  power  and  vigor  to  Byron's  poetry 
which  is  not  found  in  any  other  English  poet.  It  would  seem 
that  the  temperamental  character  of  the  poet  had  gotten  into 
these  experiments,  and  that  also  the  quieter  and  more  pastoral 
nature  of  Keats  had  not  been  omitted  from  the  experiments 
which  we  graphed  with  those  of  Byron.  It  is  admitted  that  the 
experiments  were  made  with  the  different  poets  in  mind,  and  that 
even  the  first  three  experiments  were  patterned  upon  what  was. 


ELEMENTS  OF  SPEECH  IN  RELATION  TO  POETRY         63 

conceived  to  be  the  poet's  tonal  characteristics,  but  the  results 
are  what  they  are,  and  whether  from  one  cause  or  another,  they 
show  that  the  experiments  on  Byron  called  for  the  more  activity 
on  the  part  of  the  motor  consciousness.  This  is  enough. 

The  next  experiments  to  be  considered  and  compared  are  those 
on  Arnold  and  Tennyson. 

Arnold  IV.  Nor  hi  but  smord  hiz  neerd  ukt  lod  6rn  wabez. 
From  "But  he  looked  on,  nor  smiled,  nor  bared  his  sword." 

Thinks  of   a  'battle;    (but   "smord"   never   meant   "sword"    consciously) . 
"Smord"  and  "neerd"  indicate  the  presence  of  trouble. 

Arnold  V.  Lo  mordz  iim  stur  dile  hus  ker  mize  hiz  mafe. 
From  "Like  some  old  miser  Rustum  hoards  his  fame." 

Tragic  narrative  poetry. 
"Hus"  the  highest  tone  in  the  line. 

"Maie"  not  very  pleasant;  the  auditory  consciousness  much  more  pleasant 
than   the    reading   consciousness. 

Arnold  VI.  Zi  nold  be  krem  es  laj  e  ruld  sho  hize.  From 
"  'Behold,'  she  cries,  'so  many  rages  lulled.' ' 

"Z"  the  prominent  consonant. 
"Kremlin"  aroused  by  "krem." 
Seemed  to  be  a  "begging  symphony"  of  the  Mohammedan  beggars. 

Arnold  VII.  As  she  her  echo  stormy  screams  sails  by.  From 
"Echo  her  stormy  scream  as  she  sails  by." 

"S"  the  right  sound  for  this  meaning. 

Imagery  of  excited  movement. 

The  "s"  sounds  are  very  unpleasant;  sounds  like  a  poorly  oiled  wheel. 

"Sails"  too  heavy  a  word  for  an  unaccented  syllable. 

Arnold  VIII.  All  who  pained  desert  lion  some  of  day.  From 
"Of  some  pain'd  desert  lion  who  all  day." 

Hard  to  say  "pained"  in  the  time  allowed. 

The  words  "some"  and  "desert"  disturbed  the  rhythm. 

Arnold  IX.  Her  it  the  glass  lake  flying  over  shall.  From 
"Shall  the  lake  glass  her,  flying  over  it." 

Sounds  very  pleasant;  gives  a  "thin,"  damp  feeling. 

Lips  much  in  consciousness. 

Mood  aroused  akin  to  that  of  Wlordsworth's  Lucy  poems. 

Arnold  X.    "The  sails  that  gleam  a  moment  and  are  gone." 

"Can  this  be  the  same  poet  as  the  last  nine  experiments  illustrated  ?* 
Liked  the  sense  but  not  the  sounds  or  rhythm. 


64  ROBERT  CHENAULT  GIVLER 

Arnold  XI.    "Before  this  strange  disease  of  modern  life." 

Sounds  unpleasant  per  se;  "s"  too  frequent. 

"Strange,"  very  unpleasant  and  nasal;  the  "ern"  of  modern  also  a  bad 
sound. 

Arnold  XII.    "He  lies  in  death  upon  the  common  sand." 
"Death"  as  a  sound  is  very  pleasant  here ;  one  cannot  expand  while  saying  it. 
The  words  "common  sand"  go  too  quickly  for  the  meaning. 

Tennyson  IV.  For  tharing  kern  he  toft  a  seefly  thile.  From 
"He  therefore,  turning  softly  like  a  thief." 

Thinks  of  the  ocean  and  the  surf;  "a  cynical  line." 

Means  something  soft,  quiet,  and  subdued;  "kern"  is  a  lovely  word. 

The  letter  "k,"  while  dissonant,  only  brings  out  the  harmony  of  the  other 
sounds  the  more. 

Tennyson  V.    A  whil  in  dost  ur  genward  nispers  eep.    From 
"Again  in  deeper  inward  whispers  'lost'." 
Thinks  of  a  soft,  subdued  scene;  evening. 
Thinks  of  whirling  dust;  whispers;  deep;  etc. 
Thinks  of  Gray's  Elegy. 

Tennyson  VI.  Whar  lamz  too  urking  wile  too  bisk  an  vorn. 
From  "Scorning  an  alms  to  work  whereby  to  live." 

"Vorn"  is  Scandanavian ;  thinks  of  the  early  Britons. 

"Lamz"  =  "lambs,"  but  they  were  not  frisky;  something  dark  and  weari- 
some about  the  line. 

"Urk"  brings  a  dead  stop. 

Sounds  like  Matthew  Arnold;  "urk"  a  little  hard. 

Very  personal,  and  sad;  gloomy  melancholy;  fatalistic. 

Implies  a  moral  situation. 

Tennyson  VII.    But  will  made  fate  in  weak  by  time  and  strong. 
From  "Made  weak  by  time  and  fate  but  strong  in  will." 
Uninteresting,  abstract,  philosophical. 
The  sounds  too  short  for  the  meaning. 
The  sense  takes  away  the  pleasure  of  the  sounds. 

Tennyson  VIII.  Now  crimson  sleeps  the  now  the  petal  white. 
From  "Now  sleeps  the  crimson  petal,  now  the  white." 

Imagery  of  carnations,  visual  and  olfactory. 

"Crimson"  visualized  as  "pink." 

The  sounds  are  good  even  without  meaning. 

The  sounds  of  the  word  "petal"  are  too  light  for  the  rest  of  the  line. 

"Crimson"  the  best  sound  of  all;  thinks  of  English  pudding. 

Tennyson  IX.     To  scorning  live  whereby  an  alms  to  work. 
From  "Scorning  an  alms  to  work  whereby  to  live." 
Stupid  sort  of  a  line;  "scorning"  a  dreadfully  "hard"  word. 


ELEMENTS  OF  SPEECH  IN  RELATION  TO  POETRY         65 

Laborious  combination ;  has  internal  'bodily  strains. 
Didn't  feel  that  the  tapping  was  at  all  expressive. 

Tennyson  X.    "And  on  the  mere  the  wailing  died  away." 
Rather  onomatopoetic ;  but  "died"  has  a  little  too  much  movement  about  it. 
Tried  to  be  sad,  but  did  not  succeed. 

Tennyson  XI.    "The  silent  water  slipping  from  the  hills." 
The  word  "silent"  means  distance  and  seclusion. 

"Sliding"  would  have  been  better  than  "slipping"  for  onomatopoeia;  the 
short  "i"  is  too  full  of  impact. 
Too  much  sound  for  the  sense. 
Sounds  slightly  artificial  and  banal. 
"Slipping"  is  most  annoying;  gets  no  motion  out  of  the  line. 

Tennyson  XII.    "And  all  the  coverlid  was  cloth  of  gold." 
'^Coverlid"  strangely  pleasant  for  a  "k"  sound. 

Nasal;  "cloth  of  gold"  pleasant  to  say;  but  got  visual  imagery  of  a  scarlet 
cloth. 

"Gold"  the  conspicuous  word. 
Mouth  seems  very  open  in  the  last  two  feet. 
"Lid"  doesn't  fit  in. 
Something  "insincere"  about  it. 

As  typical  of  the  numerical  results  obtained  from  the  experi- 
ments numbered  I  to  XII,  we  present  those  for  Tennyson.  First 
a  table  showing  the  results  from  the  mean  o'f  the  tappings  for 
each  subject  arranged  both  according  to  feeling-tone  and  also 
to  the  triadic  grouping  of  the  experiments  as  outlined  above.  In 
the  first  column  stand  the  abbreviations  for  the  names  of  the 
subjects,  and  in  the  last  column  the  averages  of  the  tappings  for 
the  pleasant,  unpleasant  and  neutral  experiments,  regardless  of 
group. 

The  next  table  presents  the  same  results  qua  mean  variation. 
And  the  third  table  shows  the  rank  lists  which  exhibit  the  acme 
of  the  steadiness  attained  in  this  whole  group  of  240  experiments. 
The  mean  alone  appears  to  be  significant  in  point  of  comparison 
with  the  general  psycho-motor  correlations  hitherto  obtained. 


Subject 
A.  P.  . 

TENNYSON,  EXPERIM] 

Mil 
86^ 

ENTS  I-XII.     MEAN 
IV-VI      VII-IX     X-XII 
81.6           87.4           90.0 
87.6           89.6           87.7 
93-5           •  

78.5           78.8 
75-8           79-1           

Av. 
86.3 
88.3 
93-5 

77-7 
78.3 
8^.8 

U.  ... 

N.   ..., 

B.  P. 

7=58 

U.   ... 

801 

N.  , 

81.8 

66 


ROBERT  CHENAULT  GIVLER 


C.  P  

91.8 

87.9 

83-5 

76.6 

84.9 

u  



87.3 



87.3 

N  



87.9 



87.9 

D.  P  

103.8 

1044 

102.6 

106.1 

104.2 

U  

98.8 

96.0 

97-9 

97-6 

N  





96.8 

96.8 

F.  P.    

82.0 

79-9 

83.0 

80.5 

814 

U  

77-2 

73-0 

79-6 



76.6 

N  



77.0 

80.8 

7&9 

K.  P.   . 

S6.i 

47.9 



50.8 

51.6 

U  

474 

49-1 

48.2 

N  





524 

524 

L.  P  

69.3 

67.3 

77-1 

78.5 

73-1 

U  

70.2 

80.9 

83-5 

78.2 

N  





69.0 

69.0 

M.  P.  . 

85.0 

87.8 





86.4 

U  

85.2 

80.1 

90.6 

85-7 

854 

N  



85-7 



88.8 

87.2 

P.  P. 

127.0 

131.2 

131.7 

I34.I 

131-0 

U  

127.9 



127.9 

N  

1324 





1324 

S.  P  



"3-5 

in  .3 

III.2 

II2.O 

U  

i!i5-6 





II5-6 

N. 











TENNYSON,  EXPERTS 

lENTS 

I-XII.    M.V. 

Subject 

I-III 

IV-VI 

VII-IX 

X-XII 

Av. 

A.  P  

3-7 

2-3 

4-7 

3-8 

3-6 

U  

4.1 

3-3 

3-9 

3-9 

N  



34 

34 

B.  P  

3-9 

4-3 

_^_ 

44 

4-2 

U  

7-1 

4.4 

5-8 

5-7 

N  

4-5 



4-5 

C.  P  

4-0 

4-7 

4-6 

4-2 

4-3 

U  

7-1 

M 

N. 



3-8 



3-8 

D.  P  

3-8 

1-7 

14 

2.O 

2.2 

U  

3-2 

4.6 

2.8 

3-5 

N. 



5-6 

5-6 

5-6 

F.  P. 

4-5 

6.0 

54 

4-6 

5-1 

U  

3-7 

4-5 

6.1 

4-7 

N. 

4-3 

7-2 

5-7 

K.  P.  . 

6.2 

5-3 



6.0 

5.8 

U  



6.2 

5-5 

5-9 

N  





5.7 



5-7 

ELEMENTS  OF  SPEECH  IN  RELATION  TO  POETRY         67 


L.  P  5-1             4-r            4-8            3-7 
U  3-4          3-7            3-6 
N  5-1          

M.    P  24            5.0 
U  3-9            4-8            3-3             54 
N  4-1                              4-i 

P.  ;P  1.7                 2.0                  1.6                 2.0 
N  2.6                                     
S.   P  2.0                 3.1!                 2.6 

N  2.8 

TENNYSON.     RANK  LISTS.     EXPERIMENTS  I-XII. 
Subject                                                                      Mean 

44 
3-6 
5-1 

3-7 
4-3 

1.8 

24 

2.6 
2.6 

2.8 

A  

e 

g 

f      d 

e 

g 

g 

t 

t 

g 

g 

g 

B  

c 

d 

c      e 

d 

d 

c 

d 

b 

f 

d 

b 

C  

g 

f 

g      g 

f 

f 

e 

e 

e 

d 

b 

c 

D.  

h 

(throughout) 

F  

d 

c 

d      f 

c 

c 

d 

b 

d 

b 

e 

e 

K  

a 

(throughout) 

L  

b 

(throughout) 

M  

f 

f 

>6        C 

g 

c 

t 

g 

g 

e 

t 

t 

P  

J 

(throughout) 

S.    

i 

(throughout) 

MV. 

A.  , 

d 

f 

e      g 

c 

e 

b 

g 

e 

f 

d 

h 

B  

h 

i 

g      e 

g 

f 

e 

J 

h 

J 

f 

d 

c  

b 

h 

j       f 

h 

•d 

1 

b 

d 

d 

g 

i 

D  

e 

c 

d      a 

a 

a 

i 

a 

f 

a 

a 

c 

F  

g 

g 

f      j 

J 

g 

h 

f 

i 

h 

i 

g 

K  

J 

J 

i       h 

i 

J 

f 

i 

J 

e 

J 

J 

L  

i 

d 

h      d 

c 

i 

g 

e 

g 

g 

e 

<e 

M  

f 

e 

b      i 

f 

h 

c 

c 

c 

i 

•h 

f 

P  

a 

a 

a      c 

d 

b 

a 

b 

a 

b 

b 

a 

S  

c 

b 

c      b 

b 

c 

d 

d 

b 

c 

c 

b 

Rnj 

A.  . 

e 

g 

g      d 

b 

f 

b 

f 

d 

i 

d 

i 

B  

f 

1 

e      e 

f 

g 

d 

g 

g 

J 

g 

b 

C  

b 

i 

f      h 

h 

c 

J 

h 

e 

f 

f 

g 

D  

a 

b 

b      c 

a 

b 

g 

b 

J 

a 

b 

c 

F  

g 

h 

h      g 

i 

i 

f 

d 

h 

g 

1 

h 

K  

J 

f 

J       i 

J 

,1 

e 

J 

i 

d 

i 

J 

L  

i 

d 

i       f 

d 

d 

i 

e 

f 

c 

c 

f 

M  

h 

e 

d      j 

e 

h 

c 

c 

c 

h 

h 

e 

P  

c 

a 

a      a 

g 

a 

a 

a 

a 

b 

a 

a 

S  

d 

c 

c      b 

c 

e 

h 

i 

b 

e 

e 

d 

In  the  experiments  upon  Byron  and  Keats,  subject  A.  was  the 
only  one  who  gave  longer  tappings  for  the  pleasant  than  for 
the  unpleasant  lines;  D.,  L.,  and  P.  do  the  opposites  in  both  cases; 


68  ROBERT  CHENAULT  G1VLER 

all  the  rest  of  the  subjects  are  apparently  inconsistent.  In  the 
experiments  upon  Arnold  and  Tennyson  subjects  B.  and  C.  give 
the  longer  tappings  to  the  pleasant  lines ;  D.,  K.,  M.,  and  P.  do  the 
opposite,  and  the  rest  of  the  subjects  are  again  self-contradictory, 
to  a  more  or  less  degree.  The  apparent  conclusion  is, — new 
material,  new  motor  manifestations. 

Both  of  the  rank  lists  for  the  mean  (Arnold  and  Tennyson) 
are  splendid, — indeed,  they  are  rarely  ever  so  consistent  there- 
after. But  the  rank  lists  for  the  mean  variation  and  the  range  are 
not  as  consistent  as  they  have  been  before. 

From  the  graphs  for  this  work  it  was  noticed  that  in  every 
case,  the  Tennyson  experiments  took  a  shorter  time  to  be  spoken 
than  did  those  of  Arnold.  Also  in  graphs  X  to  XII  appeared 
much  more  form  quality  in  the  Tennyson  than  in  the  Arnold 
graphs.  Even  in  the  final  average  for  all  twelve  experiments 
this  character  is  well  defined.  Again  it  is  concluded  that  there  is 
something  about  the  sounds  or  the  arrangement  of  the  sounds  in 
the  Tennyson  experiments  which  caused  the  fingers  of  the  ten 
subjects  to  move  eighteen  metres  more  while  tapping  the  Tenny- 
son experiments  than  while  tapping  those  for  Arnold. 

We  find  also  that  characteristic  feeling-tones,  moods  and  the 
like  are  correlated  with  the  type  of  motor  discharge  which  ac- 
company them.  Where  one  is  individual  and  significant,  so  is 
the  other.  Not  only  were  the  Byron  graphs  indicative  of  a 
greater  length  of  tapped  strokes  than  those  of  Keats,  and  the 
Tennyson  than  the  Arnold,  but  also  the  general  appearance,  the 
steadiness,  the  form  of  the  graph  began  at  least  at  the  tenth 
experiment  on  these  poets,  and  very  frequently  before  this,  to 
take  on  an  individuality  as  drawn  on  paper,  as  much  as  did  the 
content  and  quality  of  the  lines  as  spoken  and  understood.  So 
that  the  motor  expression  we  had  used,  namely  the  tapping 
movements  of  the  right  index  finger,  appeared  to  be  not  only 
one  that  tended  toward  automatism,  but  also  one  that  drained 
away  the  general  somatic  supply  in  a  manner  characteristic  to 
the  poet  and  fitting  to  the  purpose  in  hand.  A  fair  analogy  to 
this  is  found  in  the  case  of  singers  sympathetically  accompanying 


ELEMENTS  OF  SPEECH  IN  RELATION  TO  POETRY         69 

themselves  upon  the  piano;  where  the  two  forms  of  musical 
expression  are  identical,  at  least  functionally,  in  so  many  ways 
as  to  be  regarded  as  indicative  of  that  unity  of  the  esthetic  con- 
sciousness which  in  this  particular  instance  expresses  itself  in 
blends  of  behavior. 

To  take  up  each  poet  or  each  pair  of  poets  who  were  experi- 
mented upon  would  be  too  tedious,  and  so  we  shall  now  turn  to 
a  consideration  of  the  rest  of  these  240  experiments  only  insofar 
as  they  supply  data  for  the  main  thesis  involved  in  our  problem. 
At  the  end  of  the  series  there  appear  two  very  interesting  tables, 
which  summarize  and  focus  the  matter  of  the  correlations  in  a 
very  interesting  way.  One  generalization  is  not  out  of  order 
in  this  connection, — the  lyrical  poets,  as  every  literary  man  well 
knows,  have  employed  a  phonetic  device  which  the  other  poets 
have  not;  name  the  lyric  poets,  and  you  name  the  users  of  liquid 
sounds  in  their  poetry ;  name  again  the  lyric  poets,  and  you  have 
named  those  not  only  whose  lines  transmogrify  best,  but  also 
those  who  will  produce  in  these  tappings,  as  recorded  in  the 
graphs,  the  finer  form  quality  of  the  curve  of  the  motor  discharge. 

At  the  end  of  the  introspective  and  other  reports  of  the  ex- 
periments upon  these  remaining  sixteen  poets,  we  shall  consider 
the  graphs  for  all  of  them. 

Coleridge  I.    Thu  nase  ta  neel   ri  tarn  din  rade  thu  leer. 

Reminds  him  of  the  sound  of  cymbals. 

Sensations  cluster  around  "n"  and  "s." 

Very  highly  colored  sounds. 

"E"  is  conspicuous ;  imagery  of  the  sea. 

Stays  in  the  mouth. 

Consonants  more  prominent  than  the  vowels. 

Coleridge  IV.    Ru  thad  tha  bang  tis  leet  sta  rusk  ti  pa.    From 
"Beat  its  straight  path  along  the  dusky  air." 
Visual  imagery  of  rustic  scenery ;  simple  concept. 
Sounds  run  back  and  forth  in  the  mouth. 
Hard  to  say  "sta  rusk."    Rather  quiet  sounds  otherwise. 
Sounds  like  water  gushing  and  then  flowing  smoothly. 
Something  Russian  about  it ;  more  epic  than  lyric. 

Coleridge  V.     Ri  voze  ka  stron  sov  morst  ba  fand  thu  wame. 
From  "A  storm  of  waves  breaks  foamy  on  the  strand." 
Has  a  funereal  air ;  imagery  of  scenes  of  death. 


70  ROBERT  CHENAULT  GIYLER 

"Wame"  means  country  life. 
Images  somebody  laboring  under  a  burden. 
Felt  as  if  standing  at  bay,  in  desperation. 
Forceful ;  hard  sounds,  but  not  unpleasant. 
Sounds  like  a  strong  rebuke. 

Coleridge  VI.  Thu  zurv  les  noom  all  blether  wilden  foge. 
From  "All  golden  with  the  never  bloomless  furze." 

Some  idea  of  blessing,  or  piety  called  up. 

Seems  like  the  last  words  of  a  narrative  of  fighting. 

Weather  and  fog  and  ocean  thought  of. 

Gives  a  cold  and  lonely  feeling. 

Something  cloudy  and  wild  and  inconsistent  about  it. 

Gives  him  a  "fuzzy"  feeling;  ghost  story. 

Wordsworth  I.    Thu  rin   din  lase  ra  zeem  ti  rit  thu  tane. 
Very  natural  sounds;  gives  a  relieved,  placid  feeling. 
Too  nasal;  imagery  of  skipping  along. 
The  whole  line  a  balance  between  soft  and  loud  sounds. 
The  "t"  sound  predominated. 

Wordsworth  IV.  Thu  greem  that  brillz  toor  zong  and  hovez  a 
thane.  From  "That  nature  breathes  among  the  hills  and  groves." 

Thinks  of  the  "Lady  of  Shallott"  and  of  "Launcelot."  In  spite  of  the  "g" 
sounds,  it  seems  far  forward  in  the  mouth. 

Felt  in  the  nose;  imagery  of  fields  in  a  brilliant  green. 

"Greem"  gives  a  shock ;  "zong,"  "toor"  and  "hoves"  bring  up  bovine  concept. 

Wordsworth  V.     Ov  tathe  im  tose  ro  si  hav  lors  tal  meez. 
From  "Our  souls  have  sight  of  that  immortal  sea." 
Recalls  moral  ideas. 
Sounds  all  pleasant. 

Very  easy  to  say ;  vocalized  well ;  no  imagery. 
Once  almost  got  a  meaning  but  lost  it. 

Wordsworth  VI.    H66  kron  thu  set  with  stadz  6v  nidmg  bom. 
From  "Who  crossed  the  sands  with  ebb  of  morning  tide." 
Sounds  imiportant;  "kronz"  =  crowns. 
Epical  sounding  thing. 
The  "z"  sounds  very  effective. 

Shelley  I.    Nid  ren  dat  rul  tal  meed  run  tes  ther  mil. 
Every  foot  pleasant,  but  the  line  disconnected. 
Front  of  the  mouth  used. 

Sounds  do  not  fuse;  each  foot  is  distinct,  yet  smooth. 
Sounds  gloomy  and  melancholy. 

Surprised  because  it  went  so  smoothly;  especially  since  all  the  sounds  are 
short  and  emphatic. 

"Ther  null"  meant  "thermal,"  "diurnal" ;  very  resonant. 
The  "d"  and  "t"  sounds  predominate. 


ELEMENTS  OF  SPEECH  IN  RELATION  TO  POETRY         71 

Shelley  IV.  When  sov  thu  nesh  dol  gress  her  thon  thu  ree. 
From  "When  on  the  threshold  of  the  green  recess." 

Mediocre  poetry;  the  "s"  sounds  mar  it  again;  "sov"  makes  the  lips  come 
into  focal  consciousness. 

Very  strong  rhythm. 

Lyric  sort  of  a  thing.    Visual  imagery  of  out  doors. 

Shelley  V.  Be  quiving  theenerd  zarm  our  twiter  nin.  From 
"Quivered  beneath  our  intertwining  arms." 

Refers  to  some  very  romantic  scene. 

Something  akin  to  coquetry  thought  of;  slight  sexual  feeling  connected 
with  it. 

Arouses  many  emotions. 

"Twiter"  particularly  pleasant. 

"The  R  sounds  prominent." 

Shelley  VI.  That  flogy  166th  up  lengerz  miv  at  ron.  From 
"At  length  upon  that  gloomy  river's  flow." 

"Flogy"  seemed  sexual;  conscious  of  the  roof  of  the  mouth. 

"Flogy  looth"  is  a  lovely,  slippery  combination;  thinks  of  seaweed  on  the 
rocks. 

The  "g"  sounds  suffocate. 

Gives  that  feeling  of  uneasiness  one  has  when  shadowed. 

Marlowe  I.    Rm  ten  thu  rile  da  zade  ut  si  thi  net. 
Great  variety  of  sounds  in  it. 
Seems  to  occupy  the  middle  of  the  mouth  cavity. 
Very  little  buoyancy  or  warmth. 
Something  explanatory;  expansive  feeling. 

Marlowe  IV.     Thu  denchilz  quare  in  chevless  fanez  ov  thlre. 
From  "The  devils  there  in  chains  of  quenchless  fire." 
Feels  lips  touching  eye-teeth ;  thought  of  something  tragic  or  mock  heroic. 
Forceful  sounds;  thinks  of  "fame,"  "Macbeth,"  etc. 
Very  energetic  and  sturdy;  sounds  forward  in  the  mouth. 
Epic  line;  invites  bellowing. 

Marlowe  VI.  Thu  wurj  or  tekt  be  droot  na  ser  dra  murld. 
From  "Be  termed  a  scourge  and  a  terror  to  the  world." 

Seems  around  the  front  of  the' mouth. 

Thinks  of  the  growling  of  a  lion;  wild  and  forceful  sounds;  they  fill  the 
mouth  full. 

Too  many  consonants;  feels  like  being  whirled  about  in  a  circle  and  left 
breathless. 

Sounds  like  angry  swearing. 

Cowper  I.    Nud  ras  ti  nil  ma  zet  rin  des  du  ran. 
Tiresome,  disconnected  sounds. 
Seemed  to  be  inhibited. 


72  ROBERT  CHENAULT  GIVLER 

"If  said  low  and  quietly,  it  was  pleasant." 
Gives  an  attitude  of  confidence. 
Sounds  like  the  narrative  of  a  traveler. 

Cowper  IV.     Thu  nem  that  hife  as  van  di  meedz  az  dire. 
From  "The  deeds  that  men  admire  as  half  divine." 
Located  at  the  lips ;  kinaesthesis  the  basis  of  the  pleasure. 
Gives  a  vague  idea  of  something  confidential. 
The  "ad  van"  very  prominent. 

Cowper  V.     With  nern  i  pid  and  us  kon  ti  ra  nad.     From 
"With  mad  rapidity  and  unconcern." 
Has  a  silly  and  foolish  meaning. 

Located  forward  in  the  mouth ;  imagery  of  indefinite  activity. 
Prevalence  of  short  vowels  noticed. 
"Us"  is  peculiar  to  accent;  this  the  focal  point. 
"Pid"  balks  one;  induced  a  feeling  of  spunkiness. 

Cowper  VI.    In  shurz  re  kozement  zuv  thu  fore  he  lent.  From 
"Incurs  resentment  for  the  love  he  shows." 
Feels  it  located  back  in  the  mouth. 
Not  very  energetic;  gives  a  cool  effect. 

Milton  I.    Thu  ril  tin  leet  re  tise  da  ned  nu  ro 

Front  part  of  the  mouth  in  consciousness. 

First  two  feet  give  a  sense  of  levity,  last  two,  gravity. 

Not  emphatic,  but  most  agreeable  and  lovely;  gave  many  pleasant  thrills. 

The  "t"  and  "n"  sounds  tended  to  interchange. 

Milton  IV.  The  diz  and  tild  yoo  wis  shun  wa  mal  sate.  From 
"The  dismal  situation  waste  and  wild." 

Thought  of  the  words  "wistful  way." 

The  sounds  feel  frigid ;  imagery  of  a  cold  country. 

Very  easy  to  say ;  sounds  like  an  oration. 

Milton  V.  At  boze  he  chan  tel  mast  hiz  twilt  and  rool.  From 
"At  last  he  rose  and  twitched  his  mantle  blue." 

Suggests  early  English  romanticism;  maybe  fighting;  feels  as  if  the  front 
of  the  mouth  alone  were  used. 

Calls  up  Scotch  scenery. 

Trace  of  quiet  and  melancholy  in  "bloze";  rhythmic. 

Calls  up  some  martial  attitude. 

Milton  VI.    Me  tase  I  lawt  mi  sount  es  paze  ed  thaw.    From 
"Methought  I  saw  my  late  espoused  saint." 
Very  conscious  of  the  mouth  sensations. 
A  quiet,  peaceful  description. 

Rossetti  I.     Ni  ril  thur  dete  za  mes  rut  le  di  rin. 
Kinaesthesis   rather   forward. 


ELEMENTS  OF  SPEECH  IN  RELATION  TO  POETRY         73 

Not  musical,  but  impressive 

Felt  as  if  "mes"  and  "rin"  were  light  spots. 

Perhaps  a  description  of  nature;  "t,"  "d"  prominent. 

Rossetti  IV.    Too  nudz  ten  sprole  too  zing  this  gine  set  wes. 
From  "Tonight  this  sunset  spreads  two  golden  wings." 
Rhythmical;  imagery  of  a  sunset. 
"Too  zing"  and  "set  wes"  charming. 
'Seems  like  walking  haltingly;  five  separate  feet. 
Feels  like  setting  his  teeth. 
"Z"  and  "s"  quite  prominent. 

Rossetti  V.    Thu  wed  6v  dinth  im  ser  ish  wuble  ping.     From 
"The  wind  of  death's  imperishable  wing." 

Mysterious  and  humorous. 

Kinaesthesis  goes  from  the  throat  to  the  lips  and  back.    Imagery  of  some 
red  scene  in  nature. 

The  last  three  syllables  seemed  strangely  inadequate. 
Seems  like  climbing  a  hill  to  a  level  path. 
Warmly  emotional,  but  no  meaning. 

Rossetti  VI.     Thu  shounding  mse  thu  soudz  a  rite  thu  lore. 
From  "The  sighing  sounds,  the  lights  around  the  shore." 
More  magnificent  than  pleasant ;  visualizes  Niagara. 
Thinks  of  the  roar  of  water ;  sounds  dental  and  palatal. 
Thinks  of  an  enormous  wide  ocean. 
Description  of  an  imposing  natural  scene,  either  mountains  or  the  sea. 

Shakespeare  I.    Thu  trel  tin  rad  nur  tin  dat  si  thu  nal. 
Sounds  musical  and  rhythmic. 
Seems  to  lie  in  the  forward  part  of  the  mouth. 
Has  a  latent  meaning  of  some  kind ;  very  musical. 
Very  light  and  lyric. 

Melodious;  firm  sounds,  yet  not  obtrusive. 
"N"  and  "t"  prominent,  but  not  hard  this  time. 

Shakespeare  IV.     A  kree  zen  swev  that  zi  est  hote  in  flare. 
From  "A  crow  that  flies  in  heaven's  sweetest  air." 

Seems  all  in  the  roof  of  the  mouth ;  visual  imagery  of  a  stream,  also  of  flame. 
Too  many  "z"  and  "s"  sounds". 

Thinks  of  some  national  affair,  such  as  "conditional  dependency." 
Seems  to  be  a  description  of  a  big  fire. 

Shakespeare  V.  Thu  prime  her  kov    zal  bur  li  pa    kn  vull 
From  "Calls  back  the  lovely  April  of  her  prime." 

Kinaesthesis  not  localizable.    Every  iambic  is  an  isolated  meaning. 
Pitch  rises  to  the  middle  and  then  descends. 
Very  mysterious  and  splendid. 


74  ROBERT  CHENAULT  GIVLER 

Shakespeare  VI.    Me  kloum  hath  reet  him  f  rask  thu  now  jun. 
dom.    From  "The  region  cloud  hath  masked  him  from  me  now." 

Seems  to  describe  a  little  animal  freeing  himself. 
Kinaesthesis  elusive;  something  energetic  about  it. 
Seems  to  describe  the  frisking  of  a  clown. 
Describes  a  jolly  May  festival  on  the  very  green  grass. 
Very  amusing;  burlesque;  anticlimax; 

Spenser  I.    Rin  deer  du  ms  ta  mite  ntiz  rel  ri  deen. 
Felt  in  the  front  of  the  mouth ;  likes  R,  M,  N,  and  V. 
Hard  to  say;  feels  as  if  the  sounds  were  projected  from  the  mouth. 
Nasal,  especially  the  "d-n,1*  "n-d"  combinations. 
Queer ;  sort  of  an  old  fashioned  dignity  about  it. 

Spenser  IV.    And  ding  6v  tins  and  sa  dis  len  tul  jeedz.    From 
"And  sing  of  knights'  and  ladies'  gentle  deeds." 
Superficial  and  humorous ;  his  mouth  seemed  to  wear  a  grin. 
Couldn't  get  it  out  of  the  back  part  <of  his  mouth. 
"Z"  too  prominent. 

Spenser  V.     Be  dole  a  la  him  fidly  vur  di  sare.     From  "A 
lovely  lady  rode  him  fair  beside." 
Located  forward  in  the  mouth. 
Smooth ;  a  ballad  of  some  sort ;  love  poetry. 

Spenser  VI.    Thar  dez  el  wud  a  hap  i  choli  fi.    From  "There 
was  a  holy  chapel  edified." 

Something  rather  profound  indicated  by  it. 
Seemed  forward  in  the  month. 

Browning  I.    Nit  rase  teth  nal  ral  deet  this  mas  nus  rane. 
Emphatic  and  slightly  humorous. 
All  feels  in  the  front  of  the  mouth. 
Requires  more  mouth  movement  than  is  normal. 
Tends  to  get  a  little  cumbersome. 

Browning  IV.  And  blem  thu  vee  de  tos  ter  has  thu  mur.  From 
"And  hear  the  blessed  mutter  of  the  mass." 

Sounds  on  the  lips  mostly;  thinks  of  ocean  and  the  mermaids. 

First  amusing,  then  unpleasant. 

"Blem  the  vee"  is  swearing. 

Arouses  feelings  of  disgust,  possibly  scorn. 

Sarcastic  meaning. 

Browning  V.     He  kul  tant  ib  re  thoo  nes  pur  thu  liz.     From 
"He  threw  reluctantly  the  business  up." 

Sounds  in  the  front  of  the  mouth ;  unpleasantly  comical. 
The  sounds  do  not  go  together. 
Very  commonplace  description. 


ELEMENTS  OF  SPEECH  IN  RELATION  TO  POETRY         75 

Browning  VI.  Nad  ri  vook  il  ten  thor  nab  te  thu  son.  From 
"And  I  have  written  three  books  on  the  soul." 

Sounds  in  .the  middle  of  the  mouth. 
Certain  warm  quality  about  the  sounds. 
Seemed  to  demand  a  rapid  reading. 

Pope  I.    Ni  run  thur  til  tu  zed  ras  nus  m  rit. 
Has  a  nasal  twang. 

Tip  of  tongue  used  too  much;  not  rhythmic. 
Too  many  "n"  sounds;  gets  tongue-tied. 

Pope  IV.     Thu  shurn  6  bleke  hiz  misli  stoobing  ra.     From 
"The  sun  obliquely  shoots  his  burning  ray." 
Gives  a  cold,  unhappy  sort  of  feeling;  thinks  of  snow. 
Smooth  and  rhythmic;  thinks  of  a  windy,  sunshiny  day. 
"Nusli"  and  "stoobing"  very  bad  sounds. 

Pope  V.     Thu  loundmg  sats  in  roop  ex  tur  der  tor.     From 
"The  troops  exulting  sat  in  order  round." 
Thinks  of  a  camp  fire  and  of  baking. 
Sounds  like  a  title  to  something  odd. 
Jerky  and  full  of  irregularities. 

Pope  VI.  Thu  snoud  ant  kees  mu  sen  6s  too  thu  mes.  From 
"The  sound  must  seem  an  echo  to  the  sense." 

Thinks  of  forced  punishments. 

From  the  sublime  to  the  ridiculous,  Foot  I  to  V.  Here  the  sibilants  are 
not  unpleasant. 

Sounds  like  a  sarcastic  remark. 

Jonson  I.    Nit  Hn  thu  ta  ran  des  du  thane  ti  ril. 
Full  of  dentals;  feels  tongue  pushing  about  vigorously. 
Kinaesthesis  at  the  hard  palate  forward. 
Fairly  smooth  and  open ;  forward  in  the  mouth. 
Visual  imagery  of  bright  colors,  no  objects. 

Jonson  IV.     Or  krangther  hest  her  mat  a  ristal  rebe.     From 
"A  crystal  mirror  hangeth  at  her  breast." 
A  romantic  note  to  it. 
"Krangther"  called  up  a  blacksmith  shop. 
The  "r"  sounds  very  prominent. 

Jonson  V.    And  moice  her  vike  iz  trilpet  shroud  a  lull.    From 
"Her  voice  is  like  a  trumpet,  loud  and  shrill." 
Depressing,  heavy  and  dark;  cannot  tell  why. 
Flowing  and  easy;  visual  imagery  of  a  foggy  sea. 
Thinks  of  Vikings  and  pirates. 
Dislikes  the  tonal  anticlimax. 
Cannot  explain  why  it  should  be  so  full  of  effort. 


76  ROBERT  CHENAULT  GIVLER 

Jonson  VI.    Her  threst  ma  he  m  yoo  shand  bart  her  soor.  From 
"And  you  may  see  her  heart  shine  through  her  breast." 
Sad  and  depressing ;  gives  a  feeling  of  helplessness. 
Seems  to  be  slightly  threatening;  the  "r"  sounds  prominent. 
"Shand"  dominated  the  whole  line. 
"Threst"  very  pleasant. 
Might  well  be  a  continuation  of  IV,  and  V. 

Dryden  I.    Nu  rin  tith  16  rad  sete  ri  lid  ml  rane. 

Seems  nasal  all  through;  felt  damp  and  wet. 

Localized  exactly  in  the  middle  of  the  mouth. 

The  "n"  sounds  are  rather  nasal. 

Too  many  of  the  same  sounds  caused  inhibitions. 

Narrative,  business  like,  but  not  strong. 

Felt  as  if  he  had  been  in  a  damp  forest. 

"Tith  lo"  the  pleasantest  sound  of  them  all. 

Dryden  IV.    Too  rold  in  lure  too  stave  thu  ruse  re  to.    From 
"Resolved  to  ruin  or  to  rule  the  state." 
It  describes  some  vigorous  historical  action. 
Thinks  of  Napoleon  addressing  his  troops;  "r"  did  it. 
Located  extraordinarily  far  out  of  the  mouth. 
Gives  a  sense  of  argumentation  or  exposition. 
Suggests  melodrama  and  a  villain. 
Describes  some  one's  activity. 

Dryden  V.     Thar  krellen  zall  noos  wikerz  wade  thu  rene. 
From  "Their  cries  soon  wakened  all  the  dwellers  near." 
Related  to  a  scene  of  action;  intellectual  poetry. 
Imagery  of  some  majestic  excitement;  "r"  does  it. 
Imagery  of  a  sailing  boat,  activity ;  sounds  placed  back  in  the  mouth. 
Gets  a  sensation  of  floating. 
Imagery  of  something  flowing. 
Thinks  of  a  ship  sailing  out  of  the  harbor. 

Dryden  VI.  Di  voundz  thar  tishonz  thid  and  nod  par  bide. 
From  "And  thin  partitions  do  their  bounds  divide." 

Seems  to  describe  some  great  hurry  and  excitement.  Gets  a  ringing  in 
his  ears. 

Seems  common  and  vulgar;  the  activity  has  faded  out. 

The  last  iambic  drops  perpendicularly. 

Something  determined  and  emphatic  about  the  line. 

Swinburne  I.    Nad  ren  dor  lese  thu  ta.  rit  zede  nat  rel. 
Smooth,  and  well  placed  sounds. 
•Conscious  of  the  roof  of  the  mouth. 
Easy  to  say,  but  "zede"  disturbs. 
«T,"  "d,"  and  "r"  prominent. 


ELEMENTS  OF  SPEECH  IN  RELATION  TO  POETRY         77 

Swinburne  IV.     Duse  pawd  hits  wale  he  bluz  rath  ward  per 
sleem.     From  "Deep  sleep  hath  warmed  her  blood  through  all 

its  ways." 

Distinct  visual  imagery  of  a  fleeing  horse. 

Thought  more  and  more  of  a  blustering,  angry  man.     Rhythm  changed 
from  jerky  to  smooth  and  regular. 

Sounds  like  a  long  list  of  slang  words. 

Variety  of  tonal  effects  in  the  line. 

Swinburne  V.     And  na  wete  zare  ing  loi  sov  ting  thu  ms. 
From  "And  noise  of  singing  in  the  late  sweet  air." 

Glimpses  of  Polish  scenery  in  the  imagery. 

Suggested  some  love  scene;  serenade,  or  sweethearting. 

The  long  vowels  have  a  warmth  about  them. 

Thrills  in  the  body  at  "na  wete  zare.  .  .  ." 

Swinburne  VI.     And  rast  thu  vingyer  pand  thu  spoterd  win. 
From  "And  past  the  vineyard  and  the  water  spring." 
Feeling  of  action  and  strength. 
Kinaesthesis  very  interesting,  but  cannot  locate  it. 
"Yer"  sounds  vulgar. 
Sounds  scratchy,  metallic  and  rasping. 
"Yer"  made  the  whole  line  seem  slangy. 

Gray  I.    Thu  lere  ni  dase  ra  tarn  di  nase  tu  le. 
Rhythmical  and  smooth;  sounds  like  Scottish  poetry. 
"Di  nase"  to  nasal. 
"R,"  "t"  and  "d"  prominent. 

Gray  IV.    Too  le  thu  mawn  dup  mus  Ian  ut  thu  pon.     From 
"To  meet  the  sun  upon  the  upland  lawn." 

Visual  imagery  of  a  brook  and  a  sunrise. 

Has  a  feeling  of  eating  something  soft;  "1"  the  prominent  sound,  calls 
up  imagery  of  water. 

Imagery  of  sky,  moon  and  clouds;  sounds  rather  nasal. 
Description  of  rural  scenery. 

Gray  V.     Thu  fturperz  hekeli  zare  and  mo  yoo  sped.     From 
"And  spare  the  meek  usurper's  holy  head." 
Visualizes  a  scene  of  activity  in  the  harvest  fields. 
Rather  throaty;  thinks  of  crickets  chirping. 
Imagery  of  the  mown  hay. 
Flows  together;  visual  imagery  of  reapers. 
Describes  some  monotonous  activity. 

Gray  VI.     And  sare  thu  dez  its  wonert  staness  wete.     From 
"And  waste  its  sweetness  on  the  desert  air." 
Gives  a  tinge  of  sadness ;  does  not  imply  activity. 


78  ROBERT  CHENAULT  GIVLER 

The  line  is  as  smooth  as  water;  seems  very  dental,  too. 
Seems  like  running  down  hill;  sibilants  prominent. 
Short  "o"  and  long  "a"  do  not  fit  together. 
Could  be  easily  memorized ;  "wete"  a  very  comical  ending. 

Sydney  I.    Thu  ra  na  sit  re  vun  tith  le  nu  rale. 
In  front  of  the  mouth  more  than  in  the  back. 
Lyric  quality;  yet  somehow  hard  to  say. 
Cannot  be  said  very  rapidly. 
Tonal  warmth  about  it. 
The  vowel  sounds  predominate. 

Sydney  IV.    He  wunz  my  hone  it  woz  hiz  lart  for  zuv.    From 
"He  loves  my  heart,  for  once  it  was  his  own." 
Rhythmic  and  easy  to  say ;  narrative ;  sexual  ideas  aroused. 
Sweet  scenes  between  lovers  thought  of. 
Sounds  like  gossip. 

Sydney  V.  With  plee  ture  nazed  kon  tez  ent  case  with  prent. 
From  "With  nature  pleased,  content  with  present  case." 

Thinks  of  wooing,  pleading  and  the  like. 
Thinks  of  the  links  of  a  chain. 
"Nazed"  is  a  participle. 

Sydney  VI.     Thu  noor  no  wiz  thu  selth  re  preel  zam  perz. 
From  "The  poor  man's  wealth,  the  prisoners'  release." 
Very  smooth  and  rhythmic;  might  be  a  deprecation. 
Had  to  pause  after  selth.'" 

Of  all  these  twenty  poets,  Byron,  Keats,  Arnold,  Tennyson, 
Shelley,  Shakespeare,  Coleridge  and  Wordsworth  show  best  in 
the  rank  lists,  and  their  effect  on  the  introspective  consciousness 
was  quite  superior  to  that  made  by  the  other  eight  poets. 

The  table  next  to  be  given  needs  some  little  explanation ;  across 
the  top  of  the  page  are  written  the  abbreviations  of  the  ten  sub- 
jects who  took  part  in  the  work;  the  column  at  the  left  contains 
the  names  of  the  twenty  poets  experimented  upon.  Below  the 
abbreviations  of  the  subjects'  names  are  found  three  columns 
with  the  figures  i  and  2  in  various  positions  under  the  symbols 

P     P          U 

— ,  —  and  — .     The  numerator  of  these  verbal  fractions  is  re- 

U   N       .    N 

ferred  to  by  the  number  i  in  the  columns  below  them,  and  the 
denominator  is  referred  to  by  the  number  2  in  the  same  columns. 


ELEMENTS  OF  SPEECH  IN  RELATION  TO  POETRY         79 

P 
Now  when  the  —  column  has  a  number  i  in  it,  it  means  that  the 

U 

average  of  the  tappings  for  this  or  that  subject  were  greater  for 
the  pleasant  (P)  experiments  than  for  the  unpleasant;  when  there 
is  a  2  under  this  verbal  fraction,  it  means  that  the  unpleasant 
experiments  produced  the  greater  motor  discharge.  And  the 

P          U 

same  for  the  figures  under  the  symbols  —  and  — , — when  there 

N  N 

P 
is  the  figure  I  under  • —  it  means  that  the  averages  of  the  tappings 

N 

for  the  pleasant  experiments  were  greater  than  for  the  neutral, 
and  the  same  way  throughout  the  other  symbolic  representations. 

So  that  we  have  a  concise  summary  of  the  correlations  between 
the  feeling  tone  and  motor  discharge  for  these  240  experiments, 
with  respect  to  the  mean  of  the  tappings,  all  on  this  one  page. 
If  we  ask,  then,  who  are  the  absolutely  constant  subjects,  the 
answer  is  that  they  are  in  the  null  class;  'for  in  every  vertical 
column  we  find  the  ones  and  the  twos  scattered  all  through,  with 
only  tendencies  of  one  kind  or  another  looming  large.  Where 
there  are  no  figures  in  a  column,  it  means  that  there  were  not 
enough  different  judgments  to  make  a  correlation :  for  example, 
there  were  in  A's  judgments  on  the  affective  value  of  Keats' 
poems,  no  neutral  predicates  attached  to  the  experiments,  and  so 
on.  In  D's  judgments  on  the  Shakespere  experiments,  there 
were  only  one  kind  of  predicates  given,  and  so  in  the  columns  in 
which  there  are  no  figures  for  a  certain  poet,  we  have  slight  basis 
for  correlation. 

Following  this  page,  we  have  another  table,  which  shows  the 
same  correlations  over  again,  and  also  the  correlations  between 
the  mean  variation  and  the  feeling  tone;  the  figures  mean  the 
same  as  before,  and  here  one  can  see  a  very  much  better  correla- 
tion than  with  the  mean  alone.  This  is  the  conclusion :  that  upon 
consideration  of  the  preponderance  of  twos  in  the  first  two  col- 
umns, our  former  statement  is  again  verified,  that  it  is  not  the 
pleasant  experiences  in  these  experiments  which  call  for  the 
greatest  amount  of  motor  discharge,  but  the  unpleasant  and  the 
neutral. 


Subject   

A. 

B. 

C. 

D. 

F. 

K 

L 

M 

p 

s 

P  P  U 

P    P   U 

P   P   U 

P   P   U 

P   P   U 

PP  TT 

PP    TT 

PP    TT 

PP    TT 

PP    TT 

Poet 

UNN 

UNN 

UNN 

UNN 

UNN 

UNN 

UNN 

UNN 

UNN 

UNN 

Keats   

1 

2 

2 

2 

2 

112 

221 

2  2 

221 

211 

Byron  

1 

111 

2  1  1 

2 

112 

1 

221 

222 

221 

2  2 

Arnold   

222 

1   1  1 

1   1   1 

222 

1 

222 

222 

2 

2  2   1 

1  1  2 

Te.myson    
Wordsworth   

1 

111 

1  2  2 

2 

221 
111 

211 

222 

122 
2 

211 

222 

211 
221 

1 

1 

Coleridge  

1   1   1 

1 

111 

2 

1 

221 

2 

1 

2 

211 

122 

1  2  2 

1 

221 

2 

221 

111 

211 

2 

Shelley    

1  2  2 

221 

1 

112 

211 

2 

1 

112 

111 

2  2  1 

122 

122 

2 

121 

1   i 

2 

111 

211 

211 

222 

211 

211 

221 

2 

2 

221 

112 

1 

Shakespeare   

222 

1 

1 

222 

222 

1 

222 

1 

112 

Rossetti    

221 

1 

222 

211 

2 

112 

2 

2 

1 

Browning    

211 

211 

211 

1 

221 

221 

1 

111 

2 

112 

222 

1 

211 

211 

2 

1 

2 

2 

112 

221 

212 

122 

211 

1 

1 

2 

1 

222 

221 

122 

221 

2 

221 

111 

211 

2 

112 

222 

221 

211 

112 

121 

211 

1 

221 

122 

1 

1 

2 

1 

221 

2 

222 

111 

211 

211 

112 

2 

221 

221 

111 

122 

221 

2 

2 

221 

1 

222 

1 

112 

Gray     

122 

222 

2 

122 

221 

2 

122 

222 

2 

1 

Subj  ect    

A. 

B. 

1 

'D 

1    F 

K. 

L. 

M. 

P. 

S. 

P  P  U 

P  P  U 

P   P   U 

P   P   U 

P  P  U 

P  P  U 

P  P  U 

P   P   U 

P   P   U 

P  P  U 

Poet 

UNN 

UNN 

UNN 

UNN 

UNN 

UNN 

UNN 

UNN 

UNN 

UNN 

Keats    M. 

1 

2 

2 

2 

2 

1   1  2 

221 

122 

2  2  1 

211 

Mv. 
Byron   M. 

1 

2 

2 
222 

2 
1  2  2 

1 

2 

1 
2  1  1 

122 
1 

1  2  2 
222 

2  2  1 
2  2  1 

2  1   1 
222 

1  1  2 
1 

Mv. 
Arnold    M. 

1 

1  2  2 

1   1   1 
222 

2  1   1 
2  2   1 

2 
1  2  2 

112 
2 

1 
1  2  2 

221 
222 

222 
1 

221 
1  2  2 

2 
1  1  2 

Mv. 
Tennyson  M. 
Mv. 
Coleridge    M. 
Mv. 
Wordsworth    .M. 
Mv. 
Shelley     M. 

222 
1  2  2 
1    1    1 
1  2  2 
1   1   1 
2 
1 
211 

111 
1   1  2 
1   1   1 
1 
1 
222 
1   1   1 

111 
1  2  2 
1   1   1 
2  2   1 
1   1   1 
1   1   1 
1  2  2 
1  2  2 

222 
111 
222 
2 
2 
2 
2 
2 

1 
2  1  2 
2  2  1 
2 
1 
2  1   1 
111 
2  1   1 

222 
1  2  2 
2  1   1 
1   1  2 
221 
1  2  2 
222 
1  2  2 

222 
2   1   1 
1  2  2 
1 

2 

1 
2 
1 

2 
112 
2  1   1 
2 
1 
2  1   1 
222 
1 

221 
1   1   1 
2  1   1 
1 
2 
1  2  2 
2  2   1 
222 

1  1  2 

1 
1 
1 
2 
1 
1 
2  1  1 

Mv. 
Marlowe    M. 
Mv. 
Cowper    M. 

1  2  2 
1   1  2 
2   1   1 
1   1   1 

2  2   1 
1  2  2 
122 

221 
111 
122 
1   2  2 

1 
1 
1 
1   1   1 

1   1  2 
221 
2  2   1 

1 

2   1   1 

1 
2 
2 

2 
222 
221 
111 

1 
1   1   1 
1   1   1 

1  2  2 

1   1  2 
111 
2   1   1 

1   1   1 
1 
2 
2 

Mv. 
Milton     M. 

222 
1  2  2 

211 

222 

2   1    1 
222 

2  2   1 
1 

2 
1  2  2 

2 
2 

2  2   1 
2 

1   1  2 
1   1  2 

222 

1 
2  1  1 

Mv. 
Rossetti    M. 

221 
1  2  2 

1  2  2 
2 

1  2  2 
111 

2 

1  2  1 
2  1   1 

1 
1 

2 
1  2  2 

1   1   1 
2 

2   1   1 

1 

2  1  1 
2 

Mv. 
Shakespeare..  .M. 
Mv. 
Spenser    M. 

221 
122 
222 
2  1   1 

1 
2 
1 
2 

222 
2 
1 
2  2  1 

221 

2  1   1 
1   1  2 
222 
2 

2 
211 
222 
1 

1   1  2 
2 
1 
1 

2 
2  1   1 
222 
2 

2 
2 
1 
1  2  2 

1 
1  1  2 
112 
2   1   1 

Mv. 
Browning  .  .  .  .M. 
Mv. 
Pope    M. 

222 
112 
211 
222 

1 
1  2  2 
2  1   1 
1 

211 
1  2  2 
2  1   1 
211 

2  1   1 
2 
1 
2 

2 
11   1 
2  2  1 
221 

1 
2  1   1 
2  2  1 
1 

2 
2 
1 
2 

2 
1   1   1 
1   1   1 
2  1   1 

1  1  2 
1 
2 
1   1  2 

2  2   1 
1   1  2 
1   1  2 
222 

Mv. 

2  2   1 
222 

2 
2  2   1 

2  2   1 
2  1   1 

1 

1 

2  1   1 
2 

2 
2 

1 
2 

1   1  2 
221 

2  2   1 
1  2  2 

2  1   1 
222 

Mv. 

2   1   2 
122 

1  2  2 
1 

2   1   1 
112 

1 

112 

1 

222 

2 
222 

1 

1  2  2 

222 
222 

2  2   1 
1   1   1 

1  2  2 
1   1   1 

Mv. 
Swinburne   ...M. 
Mv. 
Gray      M. 

2  2   1 
2  2   1 
1    1   2 
122 

2 
1  2  2 
1  2   1 
1   1   1 

222 
221 
2  1   1 

2 

111 

1 
1 
1  2  2 

2  1   1 
221 
2  2  1 
211 

2  1   1 
221 
1  2  2 

2 

1   1  2 
2 
1 
2  1   1 

1  2  2 
1 
1 
1  2  2 

2  2  1 
1 
2 
1 

2  2   1 
2 
1 
2 

Mv. 

1  2  2 
122 

222 
221 

2 
112 

1  2  2 
1 

2  2   1 
2 

2 
2  2  1 

1  2  2 

1 

222 
111 

2 
2 

1 
1  2  2 

Mv. 

1   1   1 

1  2  2 

2  2   1 

2 

2 

2  2  1 

1 

222 

1 

1   1  2 

ELEMENTS  OF  SPEECH  IN  RELATION  TO  POETRY         81 

The  same  features  were  noticed  on  the  graphs  for  the  last 
sixteen  of  these  poets  as  were  brought  to  our  attention  be- 
fore;— the  more  and  more  meaning  the  line  of  poetry  con- 
tained, the  finer  and  finer  was  the  form-quality  of  the  graph  as 
drawn  on  these  plates ;  experiments  X  to  XII  for  every  poet  show 
the  same  effects  in  these  drawings, — the  first  foot  of  the  line 
and  the  last  foot  of  the  line  called  for  a  greater  motor  discharge 
than  did  the  intermediate  feet.  Particularly  irregular  were  some 
of  the  graphs,  especially  those  of  the  less  lyric  poets;  and  it  not 
infrequently  happened  that  the  rearranged  line  was  provocative 
of  a  less  regular  and  rhythmic  effect  than  the  transmogrifications 
which  preceded  it.  Indeed,  the  experiments  numbered  VII  to 
IX  were  not  very  much  enjoyed  by  the  subjects,  but,  having  begun 
that  way,  it  was  argued  as  a  better  policy  to  continue  to  the  end 
in  the  same  manner  as  we  had  begun,  so  as  not  to  spoil  the 
symmetry  of  the  work. 

With  nine  of  the  twenty  poets  experimented  upon,  the  same 
material  was  used  in  experiments  III  to  XII.  Thus  we  had  both 
introspectional  and  graphical  results  upon  the  same  tonal  content 
of  poetry  cast  into  three  forms, — transmogrification,  rearrange- 
ment, and  full  meaning.  Introspectively,  however,  the  results 
were  not  equal :  frequently  the  transmogrification  would  be  pleas- 
ant, while  the  other  forms  were  unpleasant,  and  vice  versa.  When 
this  occurred,  however,  the  graphing  showed  analogous  changes. 
And  when  the  position  of  a  strong  consonantal  combination  was 
transferred  from  one  part  of  the  line  to  another,  in  the  same  way 
the  graphings  showed  a  shift  of  accent  in  the  same  direction. 
That  tonal  replicas  were  obtained  in  the  transmogrifications  with- 
out betraying  the  meaning  was  evidenced  by  the  fact  that  in  many 
of  the  cases  the  two  forms  in  which  the  line  stood  were  asso- 
ciated together  by  the  subjects  of  their  own  accord.  Transmogri- 
fications also  always  preceded  those  lines  from  which  they  were 
taken  in  order  of  presentation. 

Insofar  as  the  graphing  of  the  81  experiments  for  those  poets 
whose  single  line  experiments  were  repeated  three  times  in  three 
different  forms  were  concerned,  the  results  showed  that  the  ex- 


82  ROBERT  CHENAULT  GIVLER 

periments  X-XII  always  took  the  shortest  time  to  be  spoken, 
which  is  quite  natural,  since  they  are  in  the  English  language ;  the 
rearrangements  take  the  next  longer  time,  and  the  transmogrifi- 
cations take  the  longest  time  to  be  spoken.  There  are  special 
cases  where  the  three  graphs  are  very  close  together,  and  again 
other  special  cases  where  they  are  very  far  apart  from  one  an- 
other; Spenser,  Cowper,  Pope  and  Milton  illustrate  the  first 
tendency,  while  the  rest  of  the  poets,  more  or  less  illustrate 
the  second.  There  are  also  great  differences  in  the  angle  of  in- 
clination of  these  graphs  from  the  horizontal  axis,  which  is  great- 
est for  those  experiments  which  were  the  most  puzzling  and  the 
most  difficult  to  recite  and  introspect  upon. 

TRANSMOGRIFICATION  OF  LARGE  PASSAGES  OF  BLANK  VERSE 

We  now  turn  to  the  experiments  concerned  with  the  psycho- 
motor  effect  of  large  passages  of  poetry.  These  are  numbered 
XIII,  XIV,  etc.  The  plan  was  as  follows:  to  find  a  ten-line 
passage  of  blank  verse  containing  nothing  but  iambics, — this  to 
be  called  No.  XIII  for  each  poet.  Experiment  No.  XIV  trans- 
mogrifies this  same  passage.  Experiments  XV  et  seq.  are  con- 
cerned with  the  effect  of  other  than  blank  verse  lines,  namely 
rhymed  passages  and  shorter  or  more  irregular  verse  forms  than 
the  heroic  blank  verse. 

It  was  very  difficult  to  construct  these  XIV  experiments;  it 
was  also  very  difficult  to  find  the  XIII  experiments; — in  several 
cases  larger  passages  than  the  ten  lines  we  used  were  boiled  down 
to  make  them,  and  often  it  was  tedious  and  slow  work ;  we  may 
have  done  injustice  to  some  of  the  poets, — certainly  now  and  then 
the  succession  of  iambics  is  doubtful,  as  in  the  Arnold  XIII  ex- 
periment. But  any  one  who  tries  to  find  ten  lines  of  blank  verse 
poetry  without  an  alteration  in  the  feet,  will  be  persuaded  at  the 
end  of  his  search  to  withhold  severe  criticism  upon  the  selections 
of  poetry  we  have  made. 

To  transmogrify  these  XIII  experiments  we  first  wrote  the 
poem  on  a  large  card  marked  out  in  small  squares,  indicating  the 
accented  consonants  and  vowels  in  red  ink,  and  the  unaccented 
in  black ;  the  card  was  then  cut  up,  a  line  at  a  time,  and  the  trans- 


ELEMENTS  OF  SPEECH  IN  RELATION  TO  POETRY         83 

mogrification  was  accomplished  by  uniting-  the  scattered  elements 
again  with  the  tonal  pattern  of  the  poem  in  mind  and  the  injunc- 
tion to  avoid  making  words  or  suggestions  of  words  in  the  tonal 
product  that  resulted. 

We  never  presented  experiments  XIII  and  XIV  for  the  same 
poet  on  the  same  day;  and  since  intervals  of  a  week  elapsed  be- 
tween the  presenting  of  experimental  material  to  the  subjects 
there  was  little  danger  that  they  would  recall  the  work  of  the 
week  previous;  these  experiments  were  presented  in  the  order  in 
which  we  give  them,  one  of  the  XIII  and  one  of  the  XIV  on  the 
same  day;  it  was  at  the  close  of  the  hour,  also,  after  the  single 
line  poems  had  been  treated  experimentally.  A  brief  period  of 
rest  was  given  before  we  attempted  this  heavy  work,  and  since 
the  subjects  had  been  tapping  for  thirty  minutes,  with  rests,  the 
practise  curve  for  the  day  was  not  likely  to  show  in  these 
experiments. 

Two  preliminary  experiments  of  this  sort  were  tried  before  it 
was  determined  to  carry  the  experimentation  in  this  direction. 
The  poets  selected  were  Coleridge  and  Keats. 

The  poem  from  Coleridge  so  treated  was  the  "Ode  to  the 
Departing  Year."  The  first  sixty-five  accented  and  the  first  sixty- 
five  unaccented  sounds  were  employed.  For  the  benefit  of  a  tonal 
comparison  of  the  original  and  the  transmogrification,  we  print 
them  both: 

Original : 

Spirit  who  sweepest  the  wild  harp  of  time! 

It  is  most  hard  with  an  untroubled  ear 
Thy  dark  inwoven  harmonies  to  hear!  etc. 

The  transmogrification : 

Thu  spard  of  teep  it  swee  mth  an  est  warp 
Mo  trime  it  zark  whoo  hild  thu  weer  uld  6v 
Ost  ib  ni  dard  wti  nar  too  rees  In  eem 
Thet  eving  clafe  on  ixtens  mong  e  mor 
I  ree  tal  cholds  en  f  eenj  end  f  rm  yad  nime 
Fren  ho  dum  stind  its  trav  erd  lind  ess  raf 
Bus  tim  its  wist  li  nond  ing  lime  thu  nart 


84  ROBERT  CHENAULT  GIVLER 

I  rad  ness  morf  ing  lart  thu  len  6v  stee 
Nith  ent  en  to  li  sile  de  wou  dess  ath 
Thu  clit  erd  mo  hent  clon  ing  sape  m  pazd 
Wha  ret  mi  flad  the  frong  mur  sol  wand  rith 
Wim  fees  I  mozd  or  yi  tent  mzd  a  moot 
Thu  hi  f  ous  hess  er  sti  em  yo  his  sul, — 

One  difference  that  will  be  at  once  noticed  between  this  and 
the  original  is  that  here  we  have  nothing  but  iambics,  while  in 
the  other  the  feet  do  not  remain  so  regular ;  neither  is  there  any- 
thing in  the  transmogrification  but  decasyllabic  lines,  which  is 
by  no  means  the  case  in  the  poem  Coleridge  wrote.  But  our 
rhythm  and  line  form  was  unchangeable  for  methodological 
reasons. 

The  subjects  were  all  handed  this  experimental  material  printed 
and  with  the  accented  syllables  marked  with  a  red  accent-stroke. 
They  were  not  told  what  it  was ;  each  one  read  it  over  until  the 
pronunciation  was  well  learned,  usually  four  or  five  times;  no 
pauses  were  allowed,  since  we  wanted  the  fresh  impression  to 
go  into  the  introspection.  When  they  felt  sufficiently  confident, 
the  lines  were  recited  and  tapped  as  all  the  previous  experiments 
had  been. 

This  is  the  introspection : 

Subject  A.  (We  shall  omit  the  feeling  tone  judgments  in  this 
series,  for  they  were  all  pleasant. )  Rather  tragical  and  solemn ; 
organic  sensations  of  a  marked  character  appeared  all  through  the 
reading;  felt  that  something  was  impending  and  tried  to  shake 
off  the  feeling,  but  couldn't;  line  four  is  the  important  line,' — 
it  is  very  descriptive  of  some  battle  or  personal  conflict;  don't 
know  exactly  what  it  means,  but  it  is  very  gloomy  and  depressing ; 
sounds  foreign  and  the  imagery  of  some  cold  climate  was  aroused. 

B.  Imagery  of  a  dark  cloudy  evening  on  the  wild  moor; 
comething    fatalistic    about    it;    wind    seems    to    be    blowing, 
some  traveler  is  hurrying  to  obtain  shelter;  auditory  imagery  of 
the  sea,  which  is  stormy  and  fierce ;  line  four  seems  to  reach  some 
climax,  and  there  is  a  secondary  climax  in  line  eleven;  I  think 
of  Schopenhauer's  philosophy  and  of  some  of  Byron's  poetry, 
but  this  is  more  sincere  than  Byron;  organic  sensations  of  a 
compelling  sort;  the  whole   feeling  is   intensely   romantic  and 
mournful. 

C.  Did  not  get  as  good  imagery  out  of  it  as  it  seemed  to 


ELEMENTS  OF  SPEECH  IN  RELATION  TO  POETRY         85 

promise ;  romantic  and  forceful ;  seems  to  be  a  description  of  some 
battle  or  of  some  scene  in  which  there  is  conflict. 

F.  Imagery  of  some  wild  scene,  maybe  at  night;  line  four  in- 
dicates some  clash  between  persons  or  things ;  very  romantic  and 
at  times  solemn  and  heavy  poetry.  The  last  line  does  not  seem  to 
belong  to  it  at  all, — the  "s"  in  "hesser"  rather  spoils  the  digni- 
fied effect  of  the  other  lines. 

L.  It's  tragic.  There  is  depicted  a  scene  of  the  middle  ages 
or  some  dark  tragedy  of  some  sort;  very  heavy  and  important 
thing,  and  someone  is  describing  either  his  own  deeds  or  those 
of  another  in  a  very  emotional  way;  some  of  it  is  soft  and 
persuasive,  and  gives  variation  to  the  heavier  parts ;  it's  all  very 
much  in  earnest;  very  full  of  activity  and  force, — might  be  from 
Othello  or  the  other  Shakespearean  tragedies. 

N.  Very  powerful  thing;  means  some  tragedy  or  heavy  and 
intense  situation ;  line  four  is  where  something  dismal  happened, — 
some  battle  described.  "Bus  tim  its  wist,"  in  line  seven 
gives  the  effect  of  something  conciliatory,  or  as  if  one  should  say, 
"Well,  I  accept  it,  if  it  has  to  be  so,"  or  something  like  that; 
gives  a  deep  sound  while  saying  it;  but  it  lightens  slightly  at 
the  end. 

T.  Very  mournful  and  sad ;  almost  oppressive ;  somebody  seems 
to  be  grieving  over  some  loss  or  some  calamity ;  at  line  four  there 
is  a  feeling  that  the  fatal  moment  is  reached  and  there  is  nothing 
to  do  but  to  endure  what  is  to  come;  the  whole  thing  sounds 
slightly  barbaric,  and  Teutonic;  might  refer  to  Scandanavia  and 
the  Norsemen ;  seems  to  refer  to  men  rather  than  women. 

W.  It  gives  a  rather  light  and  pleasing  effect  (!)  Thought 
of  a  lot  of  animals ;  seems  to  refer  to  something  other  than  my- 
self, and  to  be  out  of  doors. 

Z.  Very  dignified  and  tragical;  refers  to  some  dreadful 
calamity  and  almost  to  a  gruesome  deed  in  the  dark.  Imagery 
of  wild  country  and  wilder  ocean;  all  imagery  of  a  dark  gray 
color,  and  auditory  images  of  the  sound  of  the  waves  and  the 
wind;  highly  enjoyable  sensation  altogether;  something  seems 
to  be  inexorably  moving  and  pushing  all  before  it;  line  four  is 
where  something  fatal  happens ;  the  rest  is  not  so  tragical,  but  it 
all  seems  to  belong  together. 

Y.  Almost  doleful;  certainly  tragical  and  intensely  romantic. 
Means  much;  imagery  of  some  ocean  scene  where  a  storm  is 
raging;  everybody  is  in  a  state  of  great  fear,  and  is  hanging  on 
for  dear  life ;  this  is  in  the  first  part ;  the  last  part  is  more  hopeful. 
Perhaps  it  is  taken  from  some  one  of  Shakespeare's  tragedies. 
It  certainly  couldn't  be  any  light,  lyric  poetry. 


86  ROBERT  CHENAULT  GIVLER 

In  order  to  make  a  comparison  between  the  motor  discharge 
obtained  in  the  above  experiment  and  something  else  by  which 
it  can  be  standardized,  we  shall  take  the  next  experiment  of  this 
sort,  the  Keats-transmogrification,  which  again  every  one  of  the 
ten  subjects  found  pleasant,  though  W  only  slightly  so.  Here 
in  each  case,  we  have  no  possibility  of  correlation  between  feel- 
ing tone  and  motor  discharge,  but  only  upon  the  basis  of  what 
sounds  were  employed  in  each  experiment,  and  see  whether,  for 
example,  the  short  vowels  or  the  long  vowels  give  the  greater 
motor  impetus,  and  whether  certain  consonants  seem  to  have 
more  effect  than  others  in  this  matter. 

The  other  transmogrification  was  constructed  out  of  Keats'  "I 
stood  tiptoe  upon  a  little  hill."  We  employed  the  first  fourteen 
lines  in  which  there  are  more  unaccented  syllables  than  accented, 
though  the  accented  sound  elements  outnumber  the  unaccented, 
as  is  the  case  generally  with  Keats  as  well  as  most  of  the  English 
poets.  In  making  the  transmogrification  of  the  above  passage, 
these  supernumerary  sounds  had  to  be  omitted ;  so  did  the  rhyme, 
as  a  matter  of  course,  and  hence  the  effect  of  both  fourteen-line 
passages  of  sound  is  not  quite  the  same.  Yet  the  introspection 
seemed  to  neglect  these  differences. 

The  transmogrification  of  this  poem  of  Keats : 

Ing  lood  I  p5  pit  till  a  villy  ston 

A  stock  til  rit  sa  wand  u  sto  thu  ra 

Su  hem  wheet  bide  thu  prith  est  66ds  swich  6d 

In  woop  ul  drant  Tng  lat  a  thurv  ing  sant 

Ping  slems  li  kide  a  skleev  rand  fide  li  prat 

Thad  ston  yate  ost  oze  nar  thu  tlameds 

Morf  stawt  i  herl  thu  lod  ing  soov  thu  nor 

Nand  owds  66  merb  has  flosh  thu  cloory  porn 

Thand  whyks  creel  tesh  wu  freet  li  brom  a  slook 

Thu  swon  u  blept  the  f  ren  6v  heeldz  e  crev 

Thu  lept  less  foiz  and  oiz  nar  thit  el  I 

Thov  6rn  a  vong  ens  beel  thu  meevz  a  sil 

Thu  her?  favz  nat  sont  or  mot  thu  sood. 

In  line  eleven  one  finds  an  alliteration  "foiz  and  oiz,"  and  line 
nine  is  not  free  from  enunciation  difficulties,  but  the  above  ar- 


ELEMENTS  OF  SPEECH  IN  RELATION  TO  POETRY         87 

rangement  of  sounds  was  the  best  that  could  be  done  at  the 
time.  It  was,  for  some  reason  or  other,  much  more  difficult  to 
keep  words  from  forming  in  the  making  of  this  transmogrifica- 
tion than  in  the  case  of  the  rearrangement  of  the  Coleridge  Ode. 
This  one  took  longer,  and  more  changes  were  made  in  it  before 
it  was  finally  presented.  Rhymes  are  particularly  troublesome 
things  to  handle, — the  danger  of  alliteration  is  great,  for  they_ 
must  be  used  as  near  to  each  other  as  passible,  if  one  wants  to 
keep  the  thing  a  tonal  replica  of  the  original. 
The  subjects  gave  the  following  introspections : 

A.  Slightly  humorous  thing;  very  peculiar  sounds  reminding 
me  of  something  playfully  done;  visual  imagery  of  an  outdoor 
scene,  quite  full  of  color;  whenever  the  long  "I"  occurred,  it 
made  me  think  that  someone  was  telling  what  he  had  done,  but 
it  did  not  seem  very  important;  this  whole  combination  of  sounds 
less  solemn  and  effective  than  the  other  (Coleridge).    Something 
rather  quaint  about  it  all. 

B.  It  puts  me  in  the  midst  of  some  natural  scene,  where  there 
is  quite  a  little  animation;  nothing  heavy  at  all,  might  be  in 
spring  or  summer;  makes  my  mouth  feel  just  a  little  bit  puck- 
ered, all  the  sounds  seems  to  cause  much  movement  of  the  lips ; 
it  is  not  a  very  deep  or  profound  thing, — just  a  sort  of  playful 
and  slightly  humorous  affair;  nothing  philosophical  in  this  pas- 
sage as  there  was  in  the  other  one. 

C.  Seemed  as  though  it  ought  to  have  been  easier  to  say  than 
the  other  one,  and  it  reads  easier  to  the  eye,  but  when  I  came 
to  say  it,  it  bothered  me  more;  I  don't  get  much  imagery  out  of  it, 
but  it  all  seems  rather  light  and  gay,  in  spite  of  the  fact  that  it 
is  hard  to  read ;  so  many  unusual  and  almost  laughable  combina- 
tions in  it. 

F.  Gives  a  very  peculiar  feeling;  half  humorous  and  half 
otherwise ;  rather  romantic  and  a  little  Spencerian  in  places ;  "the 
swon  u  blept"  makes  me  think  of  Lohengrin  and  other  fanciful 
characters  of  mythology;  rather  much  ado  about  things  of  very 
little  real  importance.  Some  visual  imagery  of  pastoral  scenes, 
shepherds  and  lasses  and  the  like. 

L.  Romantic,  curious  sort  of  a  thing;  not  at  all  heavy  like  the 
last  one,  but  pleasant  on  other  grounds ;  visual  imagery  of  some 
quiet  scene  in  the  fields,  in  spring;  everything  is  just  sightly 
moving  and  nothing  very  much  is  the  matter,  though  now  and 
then  somebody  seems  to  be  making  or  trying  to  make  important 


88  ROBERT  CHENAULT  GIVLER 

that  which  is  not  so ;  parts  of  it  are  soft  and  dainty,  such  as  "thu 
leptless  foize  and  oize." 

N.  Dainty  and  light;  reminds  me  of  a  bird  cocking  its  head 
from  side  to  side  and  chirping  slightly ;  visual  imagery  of  some- 
thing fresh  and  green,  and  nothing  very  important  going  on. 
"Tiameds"  means  something  very  pretty  and  dainty,  like  an  orna- 
ment or  a  gift  of  some  kind. 

T.  Not  at  all  like  the  preceding  one;  its  all  very  light  and 
carefree,  although  some  one  seems  to  be  talking  quite  seriously 
about  it  at  times;  the  sounds  rather  too  heavy  for  the  meaning 
implied ;  I  get  plenty  of  imagery  of  things  pastoral  and  romantic, 
like  shepherds  and  flocks,  and  love  making  and  that  sort  of 
things;  "flosh  the  clury  porn"  means  that  some  one  has  found 
something  very  enjoyable  and  likeable. 

W.  Seems  to  be  quicker  than  the  other;  no  imagery,  but 
a  general  feeling  of  haste;  a  good  deal  of  motion  to  it. 

Z.  Very  curious  and  light-hearted  sort  of  a  thing;  visual 
imagery  of  a  very  fine,  clear  summer  day  and  everything  just 
right ;  attempts  to  be  serious  at  times,  but  doesn't  mean  it  at  all ; 
plenty  of  color  to  it,  and  a  great  deal  of  animation ;  seems  to  be 
all  in  the  first  person ;  some  one  is  describing  an  adventure  in  a 
somewhat  humorous  manner ;  there  may  be  laughter  in  it. 

Y.  Seems  to  be  a  description  of  some  incident  of  pretended 
importance;  very  romantic  and  quaint  sort  of  a  thing;  some  of 
sounds  are  rich  and  musical,  and  again  they  become  a  little  too 
hard  to  say  to  keep  the  impression  with  which  the  passage  started  ; 
seems  to  go  much  slower  than  the  sense  of  it  demands;  visual 
imagery  of  something  like  a  tournament  in  the  middle  ages, 
where  everybody  is  gaily  dressed  and  happy;  but  the  sounds 
seem  to  change  the  mood  in  places  where  the  appearance  of  the 
words  indicates  no  change  at  all. 

The  question  might  well  be  asked  at  this  point, — upon  just 
what  were  the  subjects  introspecting  in  connection  with  these 
transmogrifications?  That  the  passages  are  fairly  faithful  to 
the  originals  can  easily  be  discovered  by  checking  up  the  sounds 
of  both  the  versifications,  but  whether  the  subjects  were  introspect- 
ing on  Keats  and  on  Coleridge,  is  another  very  important  ques- 
tion. But  it  is  doubtful  whether  this  can  be  decided. 

The  mean  of  the  tappings  for  each  subject  indicates  that  none 
of  them  had  as  free  a  finger  movement  in  these  experiments  as 
they  had  in  those  which  immediately  preceded,  which,  again,  were 
lower  than  in  the  "Nerol"  type  of  experiment.  Evidently  the 


ELEMENTS  OF  SPEECH  IN  RELATION  TO  POETRY         89 

reading  consciousness  and  the  introspective  consciousness 
drained  some  of  the  motor  channels  of  their  usual  supply  of 
energy. 

Experiment:                                Coleridge  (Klj)                       Keats  (Stk) 

Subject  M.  M.V.        Rnj.  M.  M.V.  Rnj. 

A 60.2  1.9  ii  63.9  14  ii 

B 63.8  1.6  16  64.0  1.9  24 

C 70.3  3-6  18  71.0  4.5  15 

F 674  4.1  23  69.7  4.0  23. 

L.  64.5  4.1  13  64.9  3.1  14 

N 69.7  4.6  24  74.0  4.2  21 

T 72.1  4.8  21  76.3  3.7  17 

W 64.2  2.9  20  61.0  2.6  20 

Z 41-9  LI  13  43-6  1.5  9 

Y 36.8  1.3  9  40.0             .9 

The  rank  list  of  the  above : 

Experiment:  Klj.  Stk.  Klj.  Stk.  Klj.  Stk. 

Subject  Mean  M.V.  Rnj. 

A c  d  d  b  b  c 

B d  e  c  d  e  j 

C i  h  g  i  f  d 

F g  g  h  h  i  i 

L.   f  f  e  f  d  e 

N j  i  i  i  j  h 

T h  j  j  g  h  f 

W e  c  f  e  g  g 

Z b  b  a  c  c  b 

Y .v a  a  b  a  a  a 

The  relative  positions  are  fairly  well  kept  in  these  lists,  and 
indeed  better  than  one  might  expect  in  connection  with  such 
new  material  as  the  above  experiments  contained.  It  will  be 
noticed,  also,  that  all  of  the  subjects  but  one,  W.,  contributed 
to  the  increased  motor  output  in  connection  with  the  Keats  ex- 
periment over  that  of  the  one  on  Coleridge.  A  comparison  of 
the  tonal  elements  contained  in  these  two  experiments  reveals 
the  following  differences : 

Accented                                                 Coleridge  Keats 

Long  vowels    44  34 

Short  vowels 2P  31 

Unaccented 

Long  vowels 16  49 

Short  vowels  49  56 

Accented 

Consonants   144  152 

Unaccented 

Consonants  88  74 

Total  elements  362  356 


go  ROBERT  CHENAULT  GIVLER 

From  this  it  would  seem  that  a  preponderance  of  accented  and 
unaccented  short  vowels  together  with  fewer  long  vowels  of  both 
kinds,  more  accented  consonants  and  fewer  unaccented  conso- 
nants were  capable  of  being  interpreted  as  giving  the  greater 
-effect  on  the  motor  consciousness.  These  experiments  were  not 
given  on  the  same  days;  and  yet  it  may  be  that  the  week  that 
elapsed  between  them  for  each  subject  was  in  itself  sufficient 
to  make  the  Keats  Experiment  easier  and  more  capable  of  call- 
ing for  motor  output  than  the  other.  Yet  none  of  the  subjects 
found  the  second  of  the  presentations  easy;  each  syllable  had  to 
be  gone  over  carefully, — certainly  none  of  them  read  it  at  sight. 
Of  the  two  experiments,  the  one  on  Coleridge  "took  hold"  the 
better,  and  aroused  and  perpetuated  its  mood  the  more  easily. 

A  more  careful  examination  of  these  experiments  reveals  the 
fact  that  there  are  an  equal  number  of  accented  "h"  and  "s" 
sounds;  that  Coleridge  employs  more  accented  "f,"  "m,"  "n,"  "r," 
and  "w"  sounds;  and  that  Keats  employs  more  accented  "b," 
"d,"  "k,"  "1,"  "p,"  "st,"  "t,"  "v,"  and  "z"  sounds  than  does 
Coleridge.  The  inference  is  rather  clear,  that  the  explosive 
consonants  and  the  short  vowels  are  what  makes  the  tappings 
longer  in  the  one  case  than  in  the  other.  One  has  only  to  refer 
to  the  "Nerol"  type  of  experiments  for  the  same  sort  of  indica- 
tions; there,  likewise,  the  short  vowels  were  correlated  with  the 
greater  amounts  of  motor  discharge.  Again,  too,  the  "1"  sound 
is  allied  with  the  explosive  consonants  and  not  with  the  liquids, 
but  this  may  be  only  a  fortuitous  matter. 

From  the  graphs  of  these  experiments,  it  appeared  that  they 
were  quite  different  both  in  height  and  in  slant;  the  Coleridge 
graph  was  the  steadier  of  the  two  and  even  tended  to  sink 
slightly  at  the  end,  while  the  other  one  rose  at  one  angle  of  incli- 
nation until  the  eighth  decasyllabic  line,  and  at  another  angle 
from  then  on  until  the  end.  The  short  vowels  and  explosive 
consonants  seem  to  be  both  more  energetic  and  more  irregular  in 
their  effects  upon  the  tapping.  Also  the  greater  motor  output 
appeared  to  take  less  time. 

There  follow  the  eighteen  regular  XIII  and  XIV  experiments 
together  with  the  introspection  given  upon  them,  after  which  is 
given  the  numerical  results  and  the  various  correlations. 


ELEMENTS  OF  SPEECH  IN  RELATION  TO  POETRY         91 

KEATS,  EXPERIMENT  XIII 
I  stood  upon  a  shore,  a  pleasant  shore, 
Of  fragrance,  quietness,  and  trees,  and  flowers; 
Too  full  of  joy  and  soft  delicious  warmth. 
I  sat  me  down,  and  took  a  mouthed  shell 
And  murmured  into  it,  and  while  I  sang, 
And  with  poor  skill  let  pass  into  the  breeze 
The  dull  shell's  echo,  from  a  bow'ry  strand 
Just  opposite,  an  island  of  the  sea, 
There  came  enchantment  with  the  shifting  wind 
That  did  both  drown  and  keep  alive  my  ears. 

KEATS,  EXPERIMENT  XIV 
Too  norj  6v  zil  ti  storant  quide  a  mur 
And  shool  a  preez  6v  morp  and  thonans  la 
Pu  shoi  thu  flowd  up  weerz  i  shifting  zoor 
A  thoum  ed  frol  ger  16ft  and  tass  et  dree 
And  kafe  us  shend  a  jown  de  zang  ment  61 
And  shith  66r  wis  and  stramerd  wit  n  mant 
Shi  nowd  and  with  en  tass  I  podland  kuv 
And  skee  that  whi  zo  kul  to  dow  thu  f rim 
Thar  vi  sto  zit  in  soo  thu  strebel  nid 
Tho  chee  nam  keb  I  tid  thu  pife  me  sen. 

Keats  XIV.     (The  transmogrification  of  Keats  XIII.) 

Subject  A.  P.  Cast  a  visual  image  of  a  woodland  scene,  a  stream  in  it,  and 
then  a  great  number  of  woodland  and  water  noises  until  it  got  very  tiresome 
indeed ;  seemed  to  repeat  the  same  thing. 

B.  P.     Seemed  to  be  hard  to  say  and  there  came  imagery  of  a  summer 
scene,  and  men  in  it,  probably  engaged  in  some  contest  or  other;  it  is  a 
narrative,  anyway ;  the  "1"  sound  was  very  conscious,  and  the  accents  were 
hard  to  get  right;  yet  the  more  he  read  it,  the  smoother  it  became;  to  look 
at  the  page,  it  seems  to  be  full  of  "s"  and  "z"  sounds,  but  not  while  saying  it. 

C.  P.    Seems  to  be  a  description  of  natural  scenery,  woods,  trees,  flowers, 
grass,  sea  water  and  some  one  seems  to  be  talking  freely  about  it;  but  the 
talk  is  not  as  good  as  what  it  describes.    Kinaesthesis  seems  rather  forward. 

D.  P.     I  can't  imagine  it  being  any  thing  else  but  an  out  of  doors  scene 
that  some  one  is  describing  or  enjoying;  there's  water  there,  and  everything 
is  peaceful  and  quite  pleasant ;  it  is  emotional  and  the  feeling  is  contagious. 

K.  P.    No  exact  meaning,  or  imagery  that  was  dependable  came;  but  it  is 


92 

a  description,  and  is  just  a  little  declamatory,  with  a  note  of  melancholy,  and 
perhaps,  resignation  in  it ;  also  a  certain  amount  of  will  and  determination. 

F.  N.  Very  conscious  of  the  difficulty  in  saying  it,  and  each  syllable  felt 
as  if  it  were  a  mouthful;  no  imagery  came,  only  the  feeling  of  laboring 
at  the  pronunciation  continued  throughout.  "Podland  Kuv"  ought  to  have 
meant  something;  could  not  get  away  from  the  strain  of  reading  it. 

L.  P.  This  seems  more  intellectual  than  lyrical,  or  at  least  is  not  fully, 
freely  lyric;  such  words  as  "shilting,"  "thonance,"  "storant,"  etc.,  are  very 
pleasant;  the  sounds  ran  together  very  well.  The  tonal  quality  was  epical. 

M.  P.  It  looks  worse  than  it  sounds ;  images  a  woodland  scene,  and  thinks 
of  the  "Midsummer's  Night's  Dream";  there  is  water  in  this  scene,  and 
somthing  weird  and  peculiar  is  happening.  Would  like  to  read  it  often  and 
become  familiar  with  it. 

S.  P.  It  is  describing  a  natural  scene,  with  trees  and  flowers  and  water 
in  it;  sense  of  relaxation  in  it,  the  more  it  is  read. 

BYRON,  EXPERIMENT  XIII 
And  dreams  in  their  development  have  breath 
And  tears  and  tortures  and  the  touch  of  joy; 
They  leave  a  weight  upon  our  waking  thoughts, 
They  take  a  weight  from  off  our  waking  thoughts, 
They  do  divide  our  being;  they  become 
A  portion  of  ourselves  as  of  our  time, 
And  look  like  heralds  of  eternity ; 
They  pass  like  spirits  of  the  past,' — they  speak 
Like  sybils  of  the  future;  they  have  power — 
The  tyranny  of  pleasure  and  of  pain. 

BYRON,  EXPERIMENT  XIV 
Thu  meez  a  brent  in  zare  ing  meth  de  reet 
And  chev  thu  nad  and  theeling  porturz  vide 
Thu  droi  na  ja  tha  plake  its  sperald  here 
And  kool  dav  tulz  di  von  shun  pelz  in  toi 
In  tape  thu  wake  op  tose  our  kime  its  wa 
Morf  stawt  i  bis  tha  tuv  az  na  like  we 
Ki  tha  tur  fez  6v  nad  row  fom  be  reet 
How  kade  as  pov  ra  weet  la  kassung  fov 
Val  tur  thup  seeb  6v  pur  tha  towp  ur  thoo 
Ra  vith  66r  vol  ta  ki  thus  6v  thu  ree. 


ELEMENTS  OF  SPEECH  IN  RELATION  TO  POETRY         93 

Byron  XIV.     (From  transmogrification  of  Byron  XIII.) 

A.  N.    It  describes  action  or  scenery;  the  meaning  is  vague  and  elusive; 
doesn't  seem  to  run  smoothly  and  evenly,  and  gives  him  a  feeling  of  tension. 

B.  P.     Gives  a  feeling  of  something  portentous,  but  very  enjoyable,  even 
if  it  is  fearful;  is  quite  determined  and  full  of  energy,  and  might  be  said 
very  loud ;  it  seems  to  be  held  back  all  the  while. 

C.  P.     Quite   tiresome,   as   compared   with   the   former    (Keats) ;    but   it 
means  something,  perhaps,  like  a  struggle  or  some  activity  somewhere. 

D.  P.    Harder  to  say  than  the  one  previous  (Keats)  ;  liked  lines  4,  5  and  9 
very  much;  thought  of  something  rather  deadly  and  threatening;  the  mean- 
ing did  not  come  clearly. 

F.  P.  Goes  easier  than  the  former  (Keats).  Seems  to  be  more  creative 
than  the  former;  is  speaking  of  familiar  things  in  a  heavy  manner;  sounds 
oratorical,  and  can  be  spoken  tragically. 

K.  P.  Not  quite  so  pleasant  as  the  former ;  little  declamatory  at  first, 
later  on  is  subdued  and  a  little  tense. 

L.  U.  Seems  like  a  reporter's  account  of  some  event;  has  no  poetic  beauty; 
it  is  too  hard. 

M.  U.  Seemed  hard  to  say;  the  sounds  themselves  reminded  her  of  a 
slave  driver,  urging  somebody  on;  dreadfully  conscious  of  the  effort  to 
pronounce  it  well,  and  the  emotion  aroused  was  one  almost  of  suffering. 

P.  P.  It  was  all  hard  work  to  say,  and  about  the  middle  he  felt  as  if  some 
dreadful  force  was  pulling  him  back. 

S.  P.  The  last  was  static  (Keats)  ;  this  is  dynamic;  this  does  not  describe 
still  nature,  but  moving  nature;  there  is  no  relaxation  here  as  there  was  in 
the  last  one.  Not  so  poetically  intense,  but  more  physically  intense. 

ARNOLD,  EXPERIMENT  XIII 
And  Rustum  gazed  in  Sohrab's  face  and  took 
The  spear,  and  drew  it  from  his  side,  and  eased 
His  wound's  imperious  anguish;  but  the  blood 
Came  welling  from  the  open  gash,  and  life 
Flowed  with  the  stream ; — all  down  his  cold,  white  side 
The  crimson  torrent  ran ;  his  head  drooped  low, 
Till  now  all  strength  was  ebbed,  and  from  his  limbs 
Unwillingly  the  spirit  fled  away, 
Regretting  the  warm  mansion  which  it  left, 
And  youth  and  bloom,  and  this  delightful  world. 

ARNOLD,  EXPERIMENT  XIV 
Thu  gort  zon  krade  hiz  lanmng  wellus  fide 
And  tooz  ab  sake  it  speen  a  way  nad  zee 
Hiz  droomlng  stree  zit  nowd  thu  mith  il  grem 


94  ROBERT  CHENAULT  G1VLER 

Re  tis  in  wile  urn  stur  nad  peer  ish  f  roll 

Hiz  yoom  and  rooth  un  woun  thu  daz  it  ling 

Ka  so  shun  bli  pen  dole  and  nowb  un  thee 

Ful  theng  de  thel  war  spec  thu  sheming  lor 

Was  f  rol  do  gam  and  chi  f  land  whit  za  rud 

All  sur  ti  wib  and  weft  hiz  ti  md  mo 

And  blan  ent  streb  thu  frimz  droom  all  thu  murld. 

Arnold  XIV.     (The  transmogrification  of  XIII.) 

A.  P.    Very  highly  interesting,  like  the  preparation  for  war  or  like  some 
very  tense  and  exciting  situation;   auditory  and  visual  imagery  of  this,/ — 
also  the  sea  entered  into  the  imagery  at  times ;  the  emotional  tone,  which  was 
intense,  had  to  do  with  death  or  things  associated  with  death ;  feels  "erhabend." 

B.  P.    Attention  chiefly  occupied  with  the  pronunciation  and  the  kinaesthesis, 
which   was   sibilant   rather   than   dental;    the   passage   does   not   seem   very 
poetical  or  rhythmical;   seems  like  narrating  the  virtues  of  some  ordinary 
poetry.     (N.B. — Subject  B.  likes  very  erotic  and  sensational  poetry,  notably 
Byron  and  Keats.) 

C.  N.    Contains  a  lofty  concept,  and  is  probably  epical ;  not  very  decisive 
or  tumultuous, — at  least  it  does  not  manifest  emotions  freely;  thought  the 
sounds  were  produced  in  the  rear  part  of  the  mouth ;  was  rather  hard  to  say. 

D.  P.    Sounded  dramatic  and  oratorical;  a  trifle  tragic,  but  not  wildly  so; 
nevertheless  it  is  not  resigned ;  quite  hard  to  say,  and  seemed  to  be  full 
of  thin,  high  sounds. 

F.  P.  Slightly  laborious  here  and  there;  some  lines,  notably  Nos.  5  and  9 
went  slowly;  had  no  imagery. 

K.  P.  Slightly  rhetorical,  but  restrained  in  its  emotion ;  felt  his  own  breath 
and  pulse  quicken  at  times;  there  is  tonal  and  emotional  warmth  about  it, — 
more  so  than  with  any  previous  experiment.  It  is  not  easy  to  read. 

L.  P.  It  might  be  Tennyson  in  Swedish  or  Dutch ;  was  so  taken  up  with 
the  pronunciation  that  nothing  in  the  way  of  mood  or  emotion  came. 

M.  U.  It  dragged  horribly  and  was  hard  to  say ;  felt  as  if  the  tongue  were 
too  large  for  the  mouth ;  the  first  five  lines  began  to  mean  something,  but 
it  all  tumbled  suddenly  into  nothing  again. 

P.  P.  Felt  dreadful  tensions  in  the  finger,  which  seemed  to  be  pulling 
an  enormous  weight;  could  not  get  this  out  of  consciousness,  and  so  no- 
meaning  or  emotions  came. 

S.  P.  Describes  some  fight  or  conflict;  the  feeling  is  not  so  intense  as  it 
might  be ;  it  seems  more  internal,  more  like  giving  oneself  needless  anxieties. 

TENNYSON,  EXPERIMENT  XIII 
There  often  as  he  watched,  or  seemed  to  watch, 
So  still  the  golden  lizard  on  him  paused, 
A  phantom  made  of  many  phantoms  moved 


ELEMENTS  OF  SPEECH  IN  RELATION  TO  POETRY         95 

Before  him  haunting  him,  or  he  himself 
Moved  haunting  people,  things  and  places,  known 
Beyond  the  line;  the  mill,  the  leafy  lanes, 
The  peacock  yewtree  and  the  lonely  Hall, 
The  horse  he  drove,  the  boat  he  sold,  the  chill 
November  dawns  and  dewy-glooming  downs, 
The  gentle  shower,  the  smell  of  dying  leaves.  .  .  . 

TENNYSON,  EXPERIMENT  XIV 
Thu  stol  den  zel  ing  gade  and  mozard  ween 
Thu  len  ton  chazly  fannock  paunt  or  thill 
Be  mingz  him  choffen  mawzd  or  soovd  thu  lor 
No  shen  thu  rawnz  to  cheel  thu  gloney  fand 
And  ni  thu  sooming  vownz  and  bawntmg  nelf 
Ra  wone  thu  neevz  a  smold  him  loobry  hawn 
Thu  falton  plass  or  hemple  me  thu  sove 
Thu  dil  him  sond  6v  hote  thu  lootree  deem 
Be  forthing  glid  he  towd  thu  padri  yill 
So  deel  6v  yame  and  solgern  he  thu  vore.  .  .  . 

Tennyson  XIV.     (The  transmogrification  of  XIII.) 

A.  N.     Peculiarly  baffling  sort  of  a  description, — now  of  nature,  now  of  a 
person ;   very   much   like  a   sad   reminiscence   and    full   of   a   sombre,   wild 
melancholy;  had  visual  imagery  of   rank  nature. 

B.  U.     Seems  like  extolling  or  eulogizing  some  person  for  patience  and 
benevolence ;  did  not  seem  very  poetical ;  the  kinaesthesis  was  chiefly  dental. 

C.  P.     Interesting  description  of  something;  puts  him  in  a  mood  similar 
to  that  of  Gray's  Elegy;  there  is  some  human  interest  involved  and  some- 
thing is  at  stake.     Very  good  poetry,  and  it  runs  along  very  smoothly;  the 
kinaesthesis  seemed  to  be  more  forward  than  usual. 

D.  P.    Gives  a  feeling  of  sadness ;  dreamy  pessimism  of  a  quiet  character 
involved;  it  does  not  describe  activity,  but  has  to  do  with  some  outdoor, 
natural  scene.     Prefers  lines  6  and  8. 

F.  P.  Full  of  emotion;  visualizes  a  rich  natural  scene,  full  of  shade  and 
trees  and  water ;  somewhat  pastoral  in  aspect,  but  not  in  the  feelings  one  has 
about  it;  the  sounds  flow  together  nicely,  and  at  times  seems  a  little  like  a 
speech. 

K.  P.  It  is  melancholy  and  resigned,  and  is  not  dynamic;  tells  a  sad 
story  and  induces  tensions  and  feelings  of  restraint  at  times. 

L.  P.  Full  of  moral  enthusiasm,  devotion  and  the  like;  may  ,be  the  de- 
scription of  some  medieval  character,  knight,  or  noble  person;  very  lovely 
and  poetic. 


g6  ROBERT  CHENAULT  GIVLER 

M.  P.  Thinks  of  Chaucer's  poetry ;  interesting  and  romantic  description  of 
a  maiden  about  to  have  some  adventures;  the  words  "fannock,"  "paunt," 
and  "nelf"  very  rememberable. 

P.  P.  Gives  a  rather  hypnotic  and  dazed  feeling;  the  tapping  seems  enor- 
mously labored;  likes  line  5  the  best  of  all. 

S.  P.  Reminds  him  of  Sohrab  and  Rustum;  got  no  imagery  but  had  a 
tense  feeling  all  the  way  through;  may  be  describing  some  natural  scene. 

SHELLEY,  EXPERIMENT  XIII 
Where  plants  entwine  beneath  the  hollow  rocks 
Beside  a  sparkling  rivulet  he  stretch! 
His  languid  limbs;  a  vision  on  his  sleep 
There  came,  a  dream  of  hopes  that  never  yet 
Had  flushed  his  cheek;  he  dreamed  a  veiled  maid 
Sate  near  him,  talking  low  in  solemn  tones ; 
Her  voice  was  like  his  own,  its  music  long 
Like  woven  sounds  of  streams  and  breezes  held 
His  inmost  sense  suspended  in  its  web 
Of  many  colored  woof  and  shifting  hues. 

SHELLEY,  EXPERIMENT  XIV 
Thu  pleeth  en  tine  hiz  rolling  kossed  spar 
Be  stand  whare  hivling  nide  a  let  en  sleem 
That  skolling  stronz  6v  lanyun  buf  zu  woo 
And  chev  hiz  lois  ho  resk  and  pon  a  stree 
In  tizh  be  line  its  drame  he  miz  yod  reen 
Hiz  she  ft  sam  tewz  thar  yeng  sa  va  wid  fleek 
Li  mong  her  tush  a  cheemd  its  keetik  ho 
Hiz  breemy  vowndz  he  dawk  6v  lenmost  win 
Had  zeld  6v  wopen  shimz  him  stensed  hewm 
Wos  volerd  spo  ke  vik  set  dralerz  win. 

Shelley  XIV.     (The  transmogrification  of  XIII.) 

\A.  P.  Has  a  feeling  that  the  thing  described  is  a  rather  unfortunate  affair, 
and  deserves  sympathy ;  later  comes  the  idea  that  somebody  is  speaking  largely 
of  himself,  maybe  even  in  braggadocio. 

B.  P.    Visual  imagery  of  the  sea  shore  and  people  on  it;  a  good  deal  of 
the  "n-drone"  in  the  poem,  and  this  calls  up  the  roar  of  the  sea;  the  first 
four  lines  are  easier  to  say  than  the  last  six. 

C.  P.    Vapory  sort  of  a  thing;  seems  animistic  at  times;  got  the  idea  of 


ELEMENTS  OF  SPEECH  IN  RELATION  TO  POETRY         97 

the  sun  ishining  and  the  word  "hiz"  brought  up  rather  egoistic  notions. 
Kinaesthesis  frequently  back  in  the  mouth. 

D.  P.  Thinks  of  a  slippery  and  slidy  waterfall;  several  times  it  gave  a 
creepy,  and  crawly  feeling;  visualized  the  "Nude  descending  the  Stairs." 

F.  P.    Flows  very  well;  gives  an  outdoors,  cool  effect. 

K.  P.  Slightly  rhetorical,  but  not  heavy  or  sombre;  very  clear  and  light 
sounds,  and  it  runs  off  just  as  easily  as  real  words  do. 

OL.  U.  Cannot  seem  to  make  it  blend ;  it  doesn't  fit  into  any  organic  rhythm ; 
the  fourth  line  alone  good. 

M.  P.  It  is  first  a  narrative,  and  then  something  very  mysterious  and  like 
a  fairy  tale;  like  the  story  of  some  very  wonderful  thing,  told  with  wide 
open  eyes.  Strange  that  it  should  sound  so  foreign  and  also  so  familiar. 

P.  P.  The  first  half  is  very  good,  the  last  not ;  organic  strains  come  in  at 
the  end,  and  make  it  pull  very  hard.  "Lanion"  is  a  very  good  word. 

'S.  P.  Describes  an  adventurous  scene;  concerned  with  human  life  very 
intimately;  gets  social  concepts  about  it  and  maybe  an  idea  of  some  work  or 
activity. 

MILTON,  EXPERIMENT  XIII 

Before  the  gates  impaled  with  fire  there  sat 
On  either  side  a  formidable  shape. 
The  one  seemed  woman  to  the  waist,  and  fair, 
But  ended  foul ;  about  her  middle  round 
A  cry  of  Hell-hounds  never-ceasing  barked 
And  rung  a  hideous  peal.     The  other  shape, 
If  shape  it  might  be  called  that  shadow  seemed, 
For  each  seemed  either — black  it  stood  as  night, 
And  shook  a  dreadful  dart;  what  seemed  its  head 
The  likeness  of  a  kingly  crown  had  on. 

MILTON,  EXPERIMENT  XIV 
Thu  mor  ble  gire  be  tase  on  lape  thu  deeth 
Im  sta  thar  fide  a  shaling  'fordul  wees 
It  bowm  her  soo  mi  wendly  karb  an  tow 
But  kile  ed  mare  and  krelyus  won  er  sta 
Thu  lound  his  ningful  peem  with  shallder  sev 
It  sha  zound  bleeth  er  fung  ro  pile  a  chee 
Thu  doopness  hi  but  sta  da  nock  das  hid 
What  dees  thu  med  6v  shemd  a  cruv  had  rish 
See  tood  ma  das  a  thod  mo  kowm  if  hi 
Ne  drod  ba  tark  az  fud  re  koop  that  ree. 


98  ROBERT  CHENAULT  GIVLER 

Milton  XIV.     (The  transmogrification  of  XIII.) 

A.  U.     Gives  a  feeling  of  sombreness,  and  arouses  ideas  of  death  and 
fatalism;   makes  him  very  depressed   and  he  asked  himself  the  question, 
"what  boots  it";  consciousness  directed  toward  the  sensory  side,  and  felt 
much  strain  and  tension. 

B.  P.    Gives  an  idea  of  isome  activity,  like  the  chase,  or  even  trouble  of 
some  sort;   smooth  and  rhythmic  all  through;   head   resonance  very  pro- 
nounced, and  the  whole  mouth  cavity  seemed  to  be  active. 

C.  P.    Seems  to  be  an  exact  description  of  some  rough  scene,  maybe  of 
a  rough  and  rocky  country;  some  event  is  taking  place  at  the  present  time, 
perhaps   in  the   "historical"   present.     Kinaesthesis   not   noticed   especially; 
prefers  lines  3,  4,  and  5. 

D.  P.     It  is  not  emotional  and  not  romantic;  seems  to  be  telling  about 
some  difficult  situation;  sounds  are  hard  and  rocky,  and  yet  it  all  blends 
smoothly  together;  "d"  and  "m"  very  prominent,  and  "med,"  "des,"  etc., 
particularly  noticed. 

F.  P.  Sounds  like  a  bass  drum;  it  pounds  along  and  gives  a  feeling  of 
strain  and  force  and  sometimes  harshness,  but  as  a  whole  it  knits  together 
well;  the  numerous  "d"  sounds  are  provocative  of  strain  sensations,  and 
the  pleasure  comes  from  doing  a  difficult  task  well,  or  nearly  so. 

IK.  P.  No  very  definite  meaning  to  it  at  all,  but  in  general  it  is  weird  and 
now  and  then  cumbersome;  easy  to  say,  rhythmic. 

L.  P.  Not  a  lyric,  but  a  very  serious  and  heavy  narrative ;  describes  some- 
thing like  a  tournament,  and  the  shock  of  arms;  could  not  keep  the  excite- 
ment out  of  his  voice. 

M.  U.  Got  very  annoyed  at  it  the  more  she  said  it;  does  not  think  it 
is  at  all  lyric  or  gentle ;  too  many  "d"  sounds,  which  made  it  drag  and  scrape 
along;  for  a  while  it  sounded  like  some  of  Chaucer,  but  then  she  decided 
it  was  quite  modern;  even  belligerent  at  times. 

P.  P.  Line  5  has  a  meaning,  but  he  did  not  get  it ;  it  ought  to  mean  a  lot ; 
the  last  four  lines  pulled  like  everything  and  strain  sensations  were  felt 
all  over  the  body. 

S.  P.  It  is  a  narrative  of  some  strong  and  determined  activity  all  through; 
seems  to  be  told  in  the  first  person ;  cannot  think  it  is  lyric,  or  gentle  at 
all;  did  not  let  himself  get  into  it  very  far,  as  he  does  not  like  that  sort  of 
poetry. 

WORDSWORTH,  EXPERIMENT  XIII 
Abundant  recompense;  for  I  have  learned 
To  look  on  nature,  not  as  in  the  hour 
Of  thoughtless  youth ;  but  hearing  oftentimes 
The  still,  sad  music  of  humanity, 
Nor  harsh,  nor  grating,  though  of  ample  power 
To  chasten  and  subdue.    And  I  have  felt 
A  presence  that  disturbs  me  with  the  joy 


ELEMENTS  OF  SPEECH  IN  RELATION  TO  POETRY         99 

Of  elevated  thoughts ;  a  sense  sublime 
Of  something  far  more  deeply  interfused, 
Whose  dwelling  is  the  light  of  setting  suns.  .  .  . 

WORDSWORTH,  EXPERIMENT  XIV 
Too  lens  6v  murd  a  bunyoor  noi  dant  zi 
Nor  stellom  kree  mens  nate  ing  var  nad  merbz 
LT  pon  a  zet  for  chadden  prelling  nowr 
Hav  soolter  grenned  yote  ing  hiv  thu  kime 
Az  thawn  on  166th  ad  hovless  mi  thu  dase 
Dis  tarn  but  fewt  or  thate  ing  mewple  md 
Too  hiften  thodring  jase  ik  wes  ni  lowr 
Hew  thi  sov  paf  hooz  lawts  6  rith  e  tewd 
Sub  tel  thu  sish  a  zat  ov  wes  e  thun 
Sub  tlze  thu  spec  vom  tos  hav  mz  6v  nar.  .  .  . 

Wordsworth  XIV.     (The  transmogrification  of  XIII.) 

A.  U.  Seems  to  be  a  vague,  meaningless  jumble;  suspects  that  it  is  philo- 
sophical; certainly  it  is  not  the  description  of  any  activity,  and  does  not 
have  anything  to  do  with  the  common  acts  of  life ;  not  heroic  nor  epical. 

>B.  P.  Fairly  smooth  and  rhythmic;  mellow;  meant  something  warm  and 
pleasant;  visual  imagery  of  summer  scenes,  rivers,  and  green  shade  and 
kindred  things;  idle,  rather  than  active,  and  musing  rather  than  otherwise; 
in  spite  of  the  apparent  number  of  "th"  and  "s"  sounds,  it  was  kinaesthetically 
pleasant 

C.  P.      It  is  descriptive  of  nature,  and  has  no  climax;  runs  along  easily 
and  smoothly;  and  the  kinaesthesis  is  quite  forward. 

D.  U.    Gave  a  cold  and  clammy  feeling;  even  snaky  at  times;  it  not  active 
and  does  not  contain  anything  erotic;  imagery  of  nature,  but  not  in  summer; 
"kremense"  gave  the  idea  "cream"  =  "food." 

F.  N.  Seems  to  go  very  freely  and  easily;  many  French  sounding  words 
in  it;  no  imagery. 

K.  P.  Gets  a  feeling  of  resignation,  and  slight  melancholy;  noticed  the 
breathing  and  pulse  were  quickened  toward  the  end;  sounds  were  not  noticed 
at  all. 

L.  P.    Narrative  poetry;  slightly  elevated  and  epical. 

M.  P.  Quiet  and  subdued  in  some  places ;  but  often  the  look  of  the  words 
disturbs  the  mood;  no  imagery. 

P.  P.  Meant  nothing  as  a  whole,  though  several  of  the  words  began  to 
mean  their  phonetic  equivalent;  goes  well  and  recites  easily;  gives  the  feeling 
that  he  is  hearing  some  one  recite  something  well  learned. 

,S.  P.  It  is  like  a  description  of  (Nature,  perhaps,  of  the  sea,  the  woods, 
or  hills, — something  grand  and  lofty;  there  is  a  tinge  of  regret  in  the  last 
three  lines,  and  it  seems  to  be  mystically  said ;  went  easily  and  rhythmically. 


ioo  ROBERT  CHENAULT  GIVLER 

COLERIDGE,  EXPERIMENT  XIII 
Tis  the  merry  nightingale 
Beside  a  brook  in  mossy  forest  dell, 
That  crowds,  and  hurries,  and  precipitates 
With  fast  thick  warble  his  delicious  notes, 
With  skirmish  and  capricious  passagings, 
And  murmurs  musical  and  swift  jug  jug, 
And  one  low  piping  sound  more  sweet  than  all, 
As  he  were  fearful  that  an  April  night 
Would  be  too  short  for  him  to  utter  forth 
His  love-chant,  and  disburthen  his  full  soul 
Of  music! 

COLERIDGE,  EXPERIMENT  XIV 
Thu  rale  in  tiz  mi  brem  dus  m  zik  mur 
With  tull  est  crown  an  too  merz  ged  ri  kos 
Ja  dile  ing  mew  nad  po  gu  soof  ik  nal 
Di  f  urdz  him  sweef ul  nadriz  worthan  mewb 
With  rastful  hiz  that  tingz  az  reeble  skerth 
De  jownd  iis  list  wood  sto  ter  shandik  spec 
Wer  hile  tish  feechant  lorsej  mm  and  swo 
Be  sig  6v  ti  mor  ber  nad  few  ka  pash 
For  sil  this  tav  per  sate  lath  hiz  noo  shath 
Pre  mert  zo  ta  too  hift  en  prind  his  wun. 

Coleridge  XIV.     (The  transmogrification  of  XIII.) 

A.  U.     Got  a  conglomeration  of   feelings  and  ideas, — the  whole  mental 
state  was  disordered ;  felt  that  it  meant  something,  but  could  only  think  of 
something  to  be  prevented;  never  came  to  clear  consciousnss. 

B.  U.    First  impression  was  that  the  sounds  were  mostly  dental;  does  not 
seem  smooth  and  even,  but  jerky  and  overdone;  it  is  telling  something  in  a 
poor  way,   or  else   something  that   is   not   very  important.     "Rastful   hiz" 
brings  up  the  idea  of  conflict.    Some  of  the  accents  bother  very  much. 

C.  P.    Explanation  and  reexplanation  all  the  way  through ;  trying  to  change 
an  opinion  and  get  a  change  of  attitude;  yet  it  is  quiet  and  intimate  and 
neither  profound  nor  very  active ;  seems  to  be  an  appeal  to  the  intellect  rather 
than  to  the  emotions. 

D.  P.    Doesn't  seem  very  peaceful  or  placid;  there  seems  to  be  an  extra- 
ordinary number  of  the  "s,"  "z"  and  "t"  sounds  in  it;  the  thing  was  so  hard 
to  say,  that  no  attention  could  be  given  to  the  meaning. 


ELEMENTS  OF  SPEECH  IN  RELATION  TO  POETRY        101 

F.  P.  Pleasant  rhythm,  but  the  content  seems  dry  and  wooden;  the  sounds 
flow  together  fairly  well,  but  it  has  no  color. 

K.  P.  For  the  most  part  the  sounds  are  rather  cool  and  clear;  at  times  a 
trace  of  melancholy  enters  in;  the  sounds  are  well  arranged  and  the  rhythm 
is  uninterrupted. 

L.  U.  "Horrid !"  No  melody  to  it ;  the  sounds  do  not  blend  together,  and 
no  reaction  comes  at  all. 

M.  P.  At  first  it  sounded  "worked  over"  and  not  spontaneous,  but  after- 
wards it  got  to  be  a  quiet  little  narrative,  or  a  quiet  talk;  tries  to  be  a  little 
solemn  at  times,  but  soon  lightens  up  and  gets  almost  "pert." 

P.  N.  Means  absolutely  nothing;  some  of  the  words  tend  to  get  respelled 
and  mean  something  in  English,  German  and  French,  but  the  organic  strains 
accompanying  the  process  take  the  focus  of  consciousness;  "rastful  hiz" 
ought  to  mean  something. 

S.  N.  Nothing  suggested  or  aroused  by  it;  got  no  imagery,  nor  was  any 
sound  prominent;  it  was  just  a  thing  to  say,  and  he  was  glad  to  get  through. 

BROWNING,  EXPERIMENT  XIII 
Is  this  apparent,  when  thou  turnst  to  muse 
Upon  the  scheme  of  earth  and  man  in  chief, 
That  admiration  grows  as  knowledge  grows? 
If,  in  the  morning  of  philosophy, 
Ere  aught  had  been  recorded,  nay  perceived, 
Thou,  with  the  light  now  in  thee,  couldst  have  looked 
On  all  earth's  tenantry,  from  worm  to  bird, 
Ere  man,  her  last,  appeared  upon  the  stage— 
Thou  wouldst  have  seen  them  perfect,  and  deduced 
The  perfectness  of  others  yet  unseen. 

BROWNING,  EXPERIMENT  XIV 
The  nurnst  a  par  un  skee  that  won  ed  ra 
Shu  Ion  ant  f  erd  erz  whem  too  chorm  tho  meef 
Je  zume  ent  thur  had  lin  ra  woze  in  gris 
Now  yam  per  thul  if  dreep  iz  la  feet  rin 
Re  mord  ow  sta  them  jud  ra  seel  up  nad 
Her  staz  ing  16  feet  mee  from  ness  in  stoon 
De  gra  mi  tith  and  per  thee  vi  mf  kawm 
Thur  wan  thap  vid  now  ken  thon  dees  the  nad 
Tu  doth  her  nos  of  bew  pez  tek  of  noor 
Thu  durs  the  tep  hav  ton  thou  stib  up  tos. 


102  ROBERT  CHENAULT  GIVLER 

Browning  XIV.     (The  transmogrification  of  XIII.) 

A.  U.    Sounds  somewhat  descriptive,  and  somewhat  philosophical;  slightly 
remorseful  feeling  at  times. 

B.  U.    Not  good  poetry  at  all ;  too  stilted  and  commonplace ;  may  describe 
some  strife  or  disconcerted  state  of  mind;  very  hard  to  read,  to  say,  and 
to  tap ;  the  first  tonal  impression  was :  "ssth."    Yet  the  rhythm  seems  very 
good,  even  if  the  sounds  are  not  smooth  and  mellow. 

C.  P.    Some  parts  are  smooth  and  others  are  rough ;  seems  like  one  sound 
after  another  and  nothing  more;  the  kinaesthesis  is  very  far  forward. 

D.  U.    The  sounds  jump  around  very  irrationally;  much  impressed  with  the 
tonal  inconsistency.    No  meaning  could  be  possible  in  this  passage. 

>F.  U.  Hard  to  say;  wants  to  go  on,  but  cannot;  sounds  like  jumping  from 
one  stone  to  another  across  a  brook;  it  doesn't  flow  at  all;  to  say  some  of 
the  sounds,  e.g.  "durz"  gives  him  an  awful  pain  in  the  nose. 

K.  U.  Rhetorical  and  slightly  melancholy;  interesting  because  such  a 
dreadful  jumble  of  sounds;  couldn't  say  it  fast;  feels  as  if  he  had  bombarded 
his  face  with  words. 

L.  U.  It  is  some  soliloquy,  giving  the  pros  and  cons ;  doesn't  excite,  and  is 
not  important  or  profound. 

M.  U.  It  is  not  poetry ;  the  pleasant  sounds  are  in  the  minority ;  took  all  the 
attention  to  say  it,  and  means  nothing;  seems  like  one  hundred  separate 
syllables. 

P.  P.  Has  no  meaning,  but  goes  rather  easily;  "her  stazing,"  etc.,  ought 
to  have  some  meaning,  but  it  doesn't. 

S.  P  .  Seems  calm  and  quiet,  and  draws  some  analogy  between  nature  and 
human  life;  has  just  a  touch  of  sadness  in  it;  it  is  animistic,  and  psycholo- 
gizes ;  at  the  end  it  seems  not  sad,  but  calm. 

MARLOWE,  EXPERIMENT  XIII 
I  will,  with  engines  never  exercised, 
Conquer,  sack,  and  utterly  consume 
Your  cities  and  your  golden  palaces ; 
And.  with  the  flames  that  beat  against  the  clouds, 
Incense  the  heavens,  and  make  the  stars  to  melt, 
As  if  they  were  the  tears  of  Mahomet, 
For  hot  consumption  of  his  country's  pride ; 
And  till  by  vision  or  by  speech  I  hear 
Immortal  Jove  say,  "Cease,  my  Tamburlane," 
I  will  persist,  a  terror  to  the  world. 

MARLOWE,  EXPERIMENT  XIV 
I  sill  thu  nev  yoor  kong  en  zoom  thu  wen 
And  flazer  tex  that  guld  in  kloum  i  zelt 


ELEMENTS  OF  SPEECH  IN  RELATION  TO  POETRY        103 

Im  dile  shun  ko  for  menst  an  geeb  hiz  lane 
Thu  tamriz  wer  with  prent  a  stember  sanz 
Tha  chee  tal  spor  un  radz  yoor  sind  haz  lake 
Too  sem  thu  parz  and  ho  jmz  wav  er  zeer 
A  stith  ov  tos  bi  vizh  a  wimp  ter  zerld 
Bur  kide  iz  tif  ker  mor  nad  soo  na  lens 
Thu  hiv  erk  tov  sa  jit  per  weet  re  tesk 
Do  rut  mi  hees  6b  ta  kon  tus  H  tarn. 

Marlowe  XIV.     (The  transmogrification  of  XIII.) 

A.  P.     Thinks   of   something  in   connection   with   government   or   rulers, 
monarchies,  or  the  like;   very   egoistical,   and   is   a  conversation   describing 
something  in  utter  pride;  hard  to  say  until  this  meaning  became  focal. 

B.  P.    Predominantly  dental ;  the  rhythm  is  very  good ;  no  meaning  except 
one  of  vigorous  activity ;  thinks  of  something  high  and  bleak,  like  a  preci- 
pice and  people  near  it;  not  liquid  or  labial  enough  to  be  the  best  kind  of 
poetry. 

C.  P.    Describes  something,  possibly ;  arouses  some  attitude  of  eagerness  and 
slight  forcefulness,  and  the  kinaesthesis  was  very  far  forward  in  the  mouth 
as  if  it  was  an  oration. 

D.  U.     It  is  rough,  jerky,  noisy  and  ishallow,  and  gets  worse  at  the  end; 
seems  very  high  pitch,  and  the  "s"  is  too  prominent ;  it  is  vigor  without  depth. 

F.  P.  Has  no  harsh  sounds,  and  flows  well, — also  better  at  the  start 
than  at  the  end ;  images  some  one  reciting  very  loud,  standing  up,  and  railing 
at  the  social  order  of  things. 

K.  U.  The  sounds  get  worse  and  worse  and  the  whole  thing  is  one  em- 
phatic drive  from  beginning  to  end ;  seemed  ludicrous  on  this  account. 

L.  U.  Description  of  something,  or  else  philosophizing,  but  has  no  emo- 
tional depth;  more  or  less  interesting  as  a  collection  of  sounds,  but  there  is 
not  much  to  be  gotten  out  of  it. 

M.  U.  It  is  hard  work,  not  poetry;  gets  the  idea  that  someone  is  digging 
away  with  a  dull  shovel,  ten  pounds  of  work  to  one  ounce  of  earth;  can 
manage  the  first  five  lines  fairly  well,  but  the  rest  has  neither  rhythm,  nor 
beauty. 

P.  P.  It  means  nothing,  but  the  saying  of  it  is  a  dreadful  strain;  tried  to 
let  some  of  it  go  freely  and  then  he  got  all  mixed  up ;  and  even  when  he  tried 
to  control  it,  it  began  to  pull  his  arm  like  a  ton  of  lead;  had  to  go  slow  and 
rest  while  he  said  it. 

S.  P.  Some  passionate  and  disturbing  person  is  "bluffing"  in  this  poem; 
there  is  depicted  passion,  scorn  and  defiance. 

Rank  lists  for  the  experiments  numbered  XIII  and  XIV,  per- 
formed during  the  second  year's  work. 


IO4 


ROBERT  CHENAULT  GIVLER 


I.    THE  POEMS  IN  ENGLISH  (XIII) 


bo 


I.    Mean. 
Subject 

rt 
V 

o 
E 
Ft 

H 

2 
"o 

B 

e 

£ 

*<u 
in 

| 

i 

o 

'in 

3 

I 

— 

1 

A,  

d 

d 

c 

d 

d 

c 

c 

d 

d 

c 

B  

....      c 

c 

d 

c 

b 

d 

d 

c 

b 

b 

C  

f 

f 

g 

K 

h 

g 

£ 

h 

g 

g 

D  

....      e 

e 

e 

f 

f 

e 

e 

e 

£ 

e 

F.  

g 

g 

f 

e 

g 

f 

g 

f 

h 

f 

K  

a 

(throughout) 

L.  

b 

b 

b 

b 

c 

b 

b 

b 

c 

d 

M  

.....      h 

h 

h 

h 

e 

h 

h 

g 

e 

h 

P  

j 

(throughout) 

S  

....      i 

(throughout) 

II.    M.V. 

A  

e 

i 

f 

g 

g 

h 

e 

f 

i 

d 

B  

f 

d 

e 

f 

£ 

f 

f 

e 

d 

e 

C  

d 

e 

d 

d 

e 

g 

d 

d 

e 

f 

D  

g 

g 

g 

h 

d 

c 

g 

g 

f 

g 

F.   

.....      c 

c 

b 

c 

c 

e 

c 

b 

c 

c 

K  

j 

(throughout) 

L  

h 

h 

h 

e 

h 

d 

h 

h 

h 

h 

M  

....      i 

f 

i 

i 

i 

i 

i 

i 

g 

i 

P  

...  ..      a 

a 

a 

a 

a 

b 

a 

a 

a 

b 

S  

b 

b 

c 

b 

b 

a 

b 

c 

b 

a 

III.     Rnj. 

A  

.....      g 

h 

f 

h 

i 

i 

h 

i 

h 

g 

B  

e 

e 

e 

f 

f 

g 

£ 

d 

f 

e 

C  

d 

c 

c 

c 

d 

d 

d 

e 

e 

d 

D  

.  .  .  .  .      c 

d 

d 

d 

b 

c 

c 

a 

c 

c 

F.   

h 

i 

J 

1 

e 

e 

e 

f 

g 

f 

K  

j 

g 

i 

g 

g 

h 

g 

h 

J 

i 

L  

i 

J 

h 

i 

,1 

J 

i 

J 

i 

J 

M  

f 

f 

g 

e 

h 

f 

J 

g 

d 

h 

P  

a 

a 

a 

a 

a 

a 

b 

b 

a 

a 

S  

b 

b 

b 

b 

c 

b 

a 

c 

a 

b 

II.     THE  TRANSMOGRIFICATIONS 


I.    Mean. 

Subject 

A 

B 

C 

D 

F 


CQ 
C 

d 
h 
e 
g 


g 

CO 

>> 

C 
<u 
H 

C 

d 
h 
f 


K a  (throughout) 

L b  (throughout) 

M f         f         g        g 

P j    (throughout) 

S i   (throughout) 


J? 

| 

CO 

1 

IS 

*n 

cw 

c 

J 

<y 

•-^ 

o 

o 

*"! 

CO 

i 

3 

E 

PQ 

g 

c 

e 

d 

c 

c 

c 

e 

d 

c 

d 

d 

d 

d 

h 

g 

h 

h 

h 

f 

e 

e 

f 

e 

e 

g 

g 

f 

g 

g 

g 

ELEMENTS  OF  SPEECH  IN  RELATION  TO  POETRY       105 


II.    M.V. 

A.  

g 

g 

f 

f 

h 

h 

g 

g 

h 

h 

B  
C  

.      d 
f 

f 
d 

d 
e 

d 
h 

g 
e 

c 
f 

d 
f 

t 
e 

g 

f 

D  

i 

i 

i 

K 

c 

K 

h 

i 

e 

e 

F.   

c 

c 

c 

c 

d 

d 

b 

c 

c 

c 

K  

J 

(throughout) 

L,   

e 

e 

g 

e 

f 

e 

e 

d 

d 

d 

M  

h 

h 

h 

i 

i 

i 

i 

h 

i 

i 

P  

a 

(throughout) 

S  

b 

b 

b 

b 

b 

b 

c 

b 

b 

b 

III.     Rnj. 

A  

f 

i 

i 

J 

h 

b 

i 

J 

i 

g 

B  

e 

g 

d 

i 

i 

g 

J 

i 

J 

a 

C  

b 

f 

f 

h 

J 

e 

d 

c 

g 

i 

D  

«      d 

e 

J 

c 

K 

J 

a 

h 

e 

b 

F  

h 

b 

g 

f 

c 

c 

b 

f 

a 

t 

K  

c 

d 

e 

d 

b 

i 

h 

d 

c 

h 

L  

J 

h 

c 

e 

a 

d 

K 

g 

h 

d 

M  

g 

J 

a 

K 

f 

f 

c 

e 

b 

J 

P  

a 

a 

b 

b 

d 

a 

e 

a 

d 

e 

S  

i 

c 

h 

a 

e 

h 

f 

b 

f 

c 

CORRELATIONS  IN  POINT  OF  VOWEL  AND  CONSONANT  QUALITY, 
QUANTITY  AND  PERIODICITY 

The  rank  lists  for  these  experiments  are  the  best  we  have  yet 
obtained,  for  even  those  for  the  mean  variation  and  the  range 
show  much  steadiness  of  position  for  the  various  subjects.  The 
correlation  between  feeling  tone  and  motor  discharge,  however  is 
of  the  same  general  type  as  we  have  obtained  before;  the  unpleas- 
ant and  the  neutral  experiments  produce  the  longest  tapped 
strokes,  and  usually,  also,  the  Transmogrifications  produce  longer 
tappings  than  do  the  sources  from  which  they  were  derived. 
This  was  also  shown  by  the  graphs  for  these  experiments  which 
may  be  considered  somewhat  in  detail.  They  show  exactly  the 
same  effects  as  the  two  first  transmogrifications  did, — that  the 
explosive  consonants  and  the  short  vowels  produce  a  greater 
motor  effect  than  do  the  liquids  and  the  long  vowels;  take,  for 
example  the  first  four  of  these  experiments  performed,  the  Keats 
XIII  and  XIV,  and  the  Byron  XIII  and  XIV.  The  Keats  XIV 
graph  was  higher  than  the  Keats  XIII,  and  the  Byron  XIV  higher 
than  the  Byron  XIII;  the  XIV's  are  also  both  longer  than  the 
XIII's.  Now  take  another  point  into  consideration:  there  are 
in  the  Keats  XIV  23  short  accented  vowels,  and  40  short  unac- 


io6  ROBERT  CHENAULT  GIVLER 

cented  vowels ;  38  explosive  accented  and  22  explosive  unaccented 
consonants.  In  the  Byron  XIV  there  are  but  13  short  accented 
vowels,  29  short  unaccented  vowels,  40  explosive  accented  and 
1 8  explosive  unaccented  consonants.  The  conclusions  are  ap- 
parent and  from  the  following  lists  of  short  vowels  and  explosive 
consonants  in  these  twenty  ten-line  experiments,  one  can  see 

the  same  tendency  in  all  but  two  or  three  cases. 

Ace.  Unacc. 

Arnold:       Short  Ace.  Vowels,  26;  Unacc.,  40;  Explosive  Cons.,      37       27 
Tennyson :  Short  Ace.  Vowels,  17 ;  Unacc.,  28 ;  Explosive  Cons.,      30        18 

And  the  Arnold  experiments  aroused  more  motor  discharge  than 
did  those  of  Tennyson.  Compare  also  Shelley  and  Marlowe, 

XIII  and  XIV: 

Ace.  Unacc, 

Marlowe :  Short  Ace.  Vowels,  28 ;  Unacc.,  26 ;  Explosive  Cons.,      32        16 
Shelley:     Short  Ace.  Vowels,  22;  Unacc.,  38;  Explosive  Cons.,      36        16 

The  graphs  for  these  experiments  showed  clearly  again  that  the 
motor  discharge  is  dependent  upon  these  sounds,  and  just  as 
these  two  poets  are  nearly  equal  in  the  number  of  them  they 
employ,  so  are  the  graphs  almost  equal  in  height  and  other 
features. 

Likewise  with  Wordsworth  and  Coleridge;  the  graphs  are 
nearly  equal  in  height  and  so  are  the  determining  sounds  in 
number. 

Ace.  Unacc. 

Wordsworth:  Short  Ace.  Vowels,  24;  Unacc.,  36;  Explosive  Cons.,    31        21 
Coleridge :       Short  Ace.  Vowels,  24 ;  Unacc.,  35 ;  Explosive  Cons.,    34       24 

Milton  and  Browning  do  not  show  the  same  sort  of  correlation 
in  this  respect  as  do  the  other  poets ;  the  graphs  showed  this  very 
clearly ;  the  unlyrical  subject  matter  of  the  Browning  XIII  experi- 
ment and  the  general  negative  character  of  the  effect  of  the 
transmogrification  bring  again  into  prominence  the  introspective 
side  of  the  experiment. 

Insofar  as  any  validity  can  be  attached  to  the  results  thus 
obtained,  it  appears  that  the  accented  syllables  alone  are  not  in 
all  cases  sufficient  to  account  'for  the  increase  of  motor  output 
caused  by  one  line  or  one  passage  of  poetry  over  that  of  another. 
Construed  in  their  psycho-motor  effects,  either  syllable,  the  ac- 


ELEMENTS  OF  SPEECH  IN  RELATION  TO  POETRY        107 

cented  or  the  unaccented,  can  be  the  cause  of  heightened  motor 
manifestation.  To  this  effect,  then,  we  ally  the  results  of  our 
previous  investigation  in  regard  to  the  length  of  the  "long" 
vowels:  in  both  cases  it  appears  that  the  intentional  prolonga- 
tion of  a  letter  or  syllable  in  consciousness  is  one  thing,  and 
the  reverberant  effect  of  such  prolongation  is  another.  In  such 
cases  one  can  at  least  catch  a  curious  glimpse  of  the  functional 
nature  of  some  phases  of  the  introspective  and  motor  con- 
sciousness in  their  overlapping  parts  in  point  of  the  qualitative 
distinctions  to  be  made  between  quantitative  similars  which  only 
an  analysis  from  the  twofold  standpoint  of  psycho-motor 
manifestations  would  break  up  out  of  a  subtle  fusion. 

Mention  must  be  made  again  of  the  form-quality  of  the  graphs 
for  these  first  long  experiments.  Just  as  characteristic  differences 
had  occurred  in  the  graphs  for  the  single  lines  of  each  of  the 
poets,  so  here  the  XIII  experiment  for  any  poet  produced  a 
graph  which  had  individuality  as  contrasted  with  the  XIII  of 
any  other  poet.  Likewise  with  the  XIV  experiments.  Those 
passages,  whether  XIII  or  XIV  which  had  gone  easily  and 
smoothly  in  the  recitation  also  went  smoothly  in  the  motor  con- 
sciousness and  the  dip  of  the  graph  line  from  first  to  fifth  foot 
was  more  marked  than  in  those  poets  which  produced  other  than 
the  above  mentioned  effects.  In  every  case  the  motor  display  and 
the  introspectional  flow  showed  what  at  least  by  analogy  might 
be  called  common  parts.  Not  strange,  of  course,  since  by  this 
time  the  motor  pattern  of  consciousness  on  the  voluntary  move- 
ment side  was  now  paralleled  by  the  apperceiving  tendencies  of 
the  reading  and  speaking  consciousness. 

In  many  cases  by  actual  counting  of  the  accented  and  un- 
accented vowels  and  consonants,  it  was  not  easy  to  see  why 
some  of  the  introspective  and  motor  effects  were  produced. 
Frequently  the  very  look  of  the  page,  before  an  attempt  to 
read  it  had  been  made,  would  suddenly  "set"  the  motor  ten- 
dencies in  a  very  definite  way,  while  the  results  of  this  "setting" 
would  conflict  with  the  auditory  side  of  consciousness  at  the 
termination  of  the  experiment.  And  so  we  had  the  conflict 
of  such  things  as  the  fusion  o;f  subliminal  stimuli  for  the  read- 


io8  ROBERT  CHENAULT  GIVLER 

ing  consciousness  with  the  fusion  of  liminal  stimuli  in  the 
auditory  consciousness,  and  the  like;  here,  also,  the  position 
of  letters  in  the  line,  and  the  periodic  recurrence  of  a  letter 
as  seen,  but  neglected  in  the  speaking  consciousness,  or  of 
sounds  possibly  unpleasant  qua  sounds,  but  affording  no  dis- 
pleasure on  the  side  o:f  visual  form,  entered  as  rather  incalculable 
disturbances  throughout  the  whole  run  of  these  larger  experi- 
ments. That  they  could  have  been  made  constants,  rather  than 
variables,  however,  lies  well  within  reason,  had  the  experiments 
been  conducted  as  a  slow,  inexorable  arithmetic  of  spoken  sounds, 
rather  than  as  an  esthetico-psychological  investigation.  In  this 
connection  it  is  significant  to  remark  that  the  subjects  took  an 
entirely  different  attitude  toward  large  passages,  from  what 
they  did  toward  single  iambic  syllables,  repeated  to  the  point 
of  tedium. 

This  ended  the  experimental  work  for  the  second  year.  We 
had  performed  336  single-line  experiments  and  20  ten-line  experi- 
ments and  the  results  have  been  all  given  in  the  preceding  pages. 
On  the  whole,  the  results  are  clear;  from  the  numerical  re- 
sults of  the  128,000  tapped  strokes  made  during  this  year's  work 
we  have  obtained  proof  that  the  unpleasant  and  neutral  states 
of  mind  are  correlated  with  a  greater  motor  output  than  are  the 
pleasant  states;  from  the  introspection  on  the  vowels  and  con- 
sonant experiments  we  have  been  able  to  make  statements  about 
the  effective  and  affective  values  of  the  various  classes  of  letter 
sounds;  while  from  the  transmogrifications  of  the  large  pas- 
sages of  poetry  we  have  been  able  to  conclude  that  the  sounds  of 
poetry,  especially  lyric  poetry,  are  able  of  themselves  to  arouse 
a  mood  congruous  to  that  mood  which  the  normal  recitation  of 
the  original  poem  would  arouse.  This  is  exactly  in  line  with 
the  notion,  on  the  basis  of  which  the  original  thesis  was  made : 
the  sensational  element  in  poetry  that  is  derived  from  the  sounds 
themselves  is  immense, — poetry  is  largely  tonal, — and  it  is  cer- 
tain, conversely  speaking,  that  those  poets  which  neglect  the 
finer  sounds  of  the  language  either  deliberately  or  otherwise  deny 
themselves  a  hearing  that  is  worthy  of  cultivation. 


ELEMENTS  OF  SPEECH  IN  RELATION  TO  POETRY        109 

THE     TRANSMOGRIFICATION     OF     OTHER     THAN 
BLANK  VERSE  INTO  TEN-LINE  PASSAGES 

Only  five  subjects  took  part  in  the  third  year's  work.  With 
one  exception,  the  experiments  were  all  transmogrifications  of 
other  than  passages  of  blank  verse  poetry.  This  exception  was 
Shakespeare  XIII.  No  suitable  passage  had  been  found  during 
the  first  two  years  of  the  work,  and  this  was  the  cause  of  the 
delay.  The  numbers  attached  to  the  third  year's  experiments 
mean  as  follows:  XV  means  a  passage  of  poetry,  not  blank 
verse,  transmogrified, — usually  a  passage  in  decasyllabic  lines; 
further  experiments,  numbered  XVI,  etc.,  means  usually  a  pas- 
sage of  shorter  than  decasyllabic  verse. 

These  poems  were  not  very  successful  in  the  experiment.  In 
the  first  place,  rhyme  is  an  encumbrance  to  the  transmogrifier, — 
it  makes  alliteration  almost  a  necessity,  if  one  is  to  transmogrify 
line  for  line  or  keep  the  first  lines  of  the  poem  in  the  first  lines 
of  his  construction;  in  the  second  place,  short-lined  poetry  does 
not  have  enough  tonal  body,  usually,  to  be  satisfactorily  trans- 
mogrified into  the  heavier  decasyllabic  lines;  there  is  something 
solid  about  iambic  pentameter  which  must  be  maintained  in  order 
to  keep  the  effect  serious  and  dignified. 

We  next  give  the  introspection  of  the  third  year's  experimen- 
tation, and  after  that,  a  resume  of  the  numerical  results  and  the 
correlations  on  the  basis  of  feeling  tone  and  motor  discharge. 

KEATS,  EXPERIMENT  XV 
Second  transmogrification  of  "I  stood  tiptoe  upon  a  little  hill." 

I  hon  tie  to  thu  il  tip  ster  bi  vit 

So  lood  ing  prand  up  kide  est  mod  u  thil 

With  udz  in  bool  wuz  drant  ing  sterv  thu  slare 

A  lis  Tng  kide  u  lape  II  chist  6v  whemz 

And  fant  er  toop  ri  whemz  eet  sorn  li  neevd 

Pul  ost  a  lat  thare  din  yet  thob  thu  dar 

From  f I  li  kov  the  ept  had  mawt  thu  tost 

Az  eur  thoze  klorn  and  flowdz  thare  pite  neu  slere 

Thu  brox  li  frook  swand  kresh  from  skete  eu  swen 

Bland  ev  ringz  kler  tha  hept  wer  shon  thu  eeldz. 


i io  ROBERT  CHENAULT  GIVLER 

This  passage  contains  the  first  fifty  accented  and  the  first 
fifty  unaccented  syllables  of  the  poem  from  which  it  was  taken. 

INTROSPECTION 

B.  P.  Flows  well,  and  the  rhythm  is  regular  and  satisfactory;  seems  to 
be  telling  some  tale,  perhaps  slightly  epical  in  nature.  Upon  reading  it  a 
second  time,  got  imagery  of  a  shore  and  a  chivalric  or  romantic  scene;  the 
setting  may  be  slightly  sexual  in  its  significance. 

F.  P.  The  most  pleasant  lines  are  Nos,  6  and  n;  makes  him  think  of 
Anglo  Saxon  poetry;  imagery  of  an  open  air  scene,  with  sunshine  and 
flowers ;  some  of  the  sounds  appear  remarkably  strong  for  such  a  description. 

K.  P.  Sounds  a  little  mysterious  and  melancholy;  thinks  of  English 
country  scenery  in  the  fall  of  the  year, — certainly  not  in  the  winter;  rather 
warm,  tonally,  but  not  at  all  deep;  felt  the  pitch  to  sink  a  trifle  toward  the 
end;  surprised  that  it  went  even  as  well  as  it  did;  kinaesthesis  not  noticed. 

iL.  P.  Recalls  the  days  of  chivalry,  and  thinks  of  King  Arthur;  gives 
a  thrill  in  the  breast  to  read  it,  and  it  all  goes  very  smoothly ;  felt  the  accent 
to  be  very  prominent  and  expressive;  it  sounds  familiar,  but  he  cannot  tell 
when  or  where  he  has  seen  it  before. 

M.  P.  Seems  very  long;  at  first  the  imagery  was  that  of  a  ship  and 
the  sound  of  the  water  and  the  calls  of  the  sailors,  but  later  it  changed 
to  a  more  subtle,  and  very  delicate  thing  like  some  romantic  scene  and  idle 
and  care-free  people;  the  consciousness  was  a  sound-consciousness  entirely, 
with  the  exception  of  fleeting  visual  imagery,  and  the  tapping  was  forgotten 
entirely. 

BYRON,  EXPERIMENT  XV 

The  transmogrification  of  the  "Apostrophe  to  the  Ocean,"  be- 
ginning with  "There  is  a  pleasure  in  the  pathless  woods,"  and 
ending  with  the  fiftieth  accented  and  the  fiftieth  unaccented 
syllable. 

Thu  plare  less  win  Tz  ath  66r  loodz  u  pezh 
U  ron  ture  thare  H  nore  thu  pash  iz  trone 
So  thon  whare  oodz  in  si  iz  thare  zik  rin 
And  meu  dits  bi  thu  nore  tepe  Ian  e  se 
Ni  muv  thu  wha  f rin  eel  ter  chim  but  lor 
I  stm  thot  meez  ure  thes  our  fing  be  vail 
I  pran  thu  win  gle  mth  6t  or  kon  soo 
And  kes  too  yare  ket  all  rex  tid  6m  do 
L66  bele  an  kar  yole  I  not  on  band  epe. 


ELEMENTS  OF  SPEECH  IN  RELATION  TO  POETRY        in 

INTROSPECTION 

B.  P.  From  the  n-drone  the  passage  contains  he  gets  the  idea  of  a  forest 
and  ocean  scene,  rather  peaceful,  languid  and  quiet ;  it  may  be  that  some  one  is 
philosophizing  or  lecturing  on  contentment;  quite  a  lot  of  sibilants  in  the 
passage  and  frequently  "v"  and  "f,"  also  the  liquids. 

F.  P.  Had  quite  a  good  deal  of  difficulty  in  saying  it;  and  doesn't  feel 
that  the  ending  is  satisfactory  at  all;  but  the  passage  is  strong  and  vigorous, 
with  a  sort  of  latent  strength  and  it  gives  him  the  idea  of  something  semi- 
heroic.  The  staccato  effect  of  "k"  is  noticed  frequently. 

K.  P.  Gets  a  vivid  image  of  the  sea-shore  on  a  cool  day ;  melancholy  enters 
into  the  whole  concept,  which  is  not  gotten  by  any  association,  but  by  a 
direct  evocation  from  the  sounds ;  thinks  the  passage  is  homogenous  in  sound 
effects,  and  ability  to  call  up  these  images  and  notions. 

L.  U.  (Subject  slightly  weary.)  Nearly  every  line  contains  some  objec- 
tionable sound;  "epe"  in  the  last  line  does  not  end  the  passage  properly 
(this  passage  was  presented  again  later  to  the  same  subject,  he  remembered 
not  having  liked  it  and  the  experiment  was  not  a  success.) 

M.  P.  Imagery  of  a  meadow  enveloped  by  a  mid-summer  mist;  slightly 
chilly  feeling  accompanied  the  imagery;  the  saying  of  the  lines  was  rather 
difficult,  and  this  took  all  the  attention. 

GRAY,  EXPERIMENT  XV 

Transmogrification  of  the  first  fifty  accented  and  unaccented 
syllables  of  the  "Elegy." 

Thu  nart  ing  kel  6v  per  thu  doll  sle 

Thu  lerd  an  a  f  mg  plo  thu  ta  mand  plor 

Hiz  6m  ri  we  zined  olz  eu  modz  thu  we 

Too  low  nes  wark  too  erld  thu  land  erd  devez 

Thu  himmer  glite  wing  ladez  and  hil  zow  fon 

Pa  stol  men  dolez  thu  sail  nes  and  thu  sare 

Hiz  bav  ing  sontle  whe  thu  tl  ware  dreelz 

Thu  siddant  drow  lisk  till  tingz  flave  at  klm 

Whan  yoldz  from  hontled  mour  vT  mowlmg  tane 

Thu  po  kom  dife  thu  san  tu  muz  der  ploon. 

INTROSPECTION 

B.  P.  Describes  something  that  happened  long  ago  and  gives  a  feeling  of 
content,  rest  and  solace;  imagery  of  romantic  country  scenery,  and  now  and 
then  sees  an  old  man  moving,  but  not  vigorously;  very  rich  imagery  and  he 
becomes  totally  empathic  to  the  scene.  The  sounds  seemed  at  first  rough, 
but  afterwards  smoothed  down  very  much. 

F.  P.    Arouses  a  sober  mood,  in  spite  of  the  fact  that  the  sounds  now  and 


ii2  ROBERT  CHENAULT  GIVLER 

then  are  dreadfully  turgid;  gets  visual  imagery  of  the  outdoors,  but  it  is  not 
very  clear. 

K.  P.  Seems  hard  to  say;  a  very  definite  mood  seems  to  be  latent  in  it; 
thinks  of  the  fall  of  the  year  and  the  woods,  and  now  and  then  a  trace  of 
melancholy  enters  into  it;  now  and  then  >he  thought  of  summer  instead  of  the 
fall,  but  it  changed  again  and  ended  in  the  latter  season. 

L.  'U.  Certainly  it  is  not  dramatic ;  it  sounds  rather  sleepy  and  ineffective 
(subject  does  not  greatly  care  for  Elegy)  ;  got  no  imagery  and  did  not 
find  it  easy  to  say;  calls  it  "inconsequential." 

M.  U.  It  "looks"  bad,  and  is  hard  to  say;  too  many  "z"  sounds  in  it,  but 
cannot  tell  why;  gets  ideas  of  lazy  people  and  stupid  foreigners, — people 
that  are  not  up  and  doing  (this  kind  of  human  beings  are  "persona  non 
grata"  to  subject  M). 

BROWNING,  EXPERIMENT  XV 
Transmogrification  of  of  "Rabbi  Ben  Ezra,"  lines  I  to  13^2. 

Thu  jold  a  mest  iz  bong  wuz  et  wo  be 

Too  lerst  6v  whade  thu  fe  grith  ife  thu  chim 

For  hm  iz  tast  I  leth  ar  plimz  a  ge 

H66  f  role  ust  whade  our  shand  66th  saf  e  boze 

Ya  nail  tring  flod  that  has  but  mand  ich  reev 

Re  nowrz  ha  boze  ich  mot  is  ken  mand  thil 

Nor  est  whake  sail  traz  Hde  6m  jot  sing  mowrz 

It  larz  whem  hov  that  mernd  66th  stire  which  flendz 

Be  ma  theurd  sig  yan  blarz  nor  fendz  ad  nail 

Nor  hi  chand  nopes  us  tof  6v  nul  fan  zeer. 

INTROSPECTION 

B.  P.  Seems  fairly  rhythmic  and  easy;  kinaesthesis  is  everywhere  in  the 
mouth;  evokes  a  mood  of  matter  of  fact  pessimism;  thinks  of  some  middle- 
aged  person,  some  pseudo-philosopher  advising  a  younger  person;  is  sophisti- 
cated, disillusioned  and  resigned. 

F.  P.  Sort  of  humorously  eloquent ;  almost  physically  ticklish ;  now  and 
then  a  slightly  tragical  feeling,  but  laughed  at  it;  the  sounds  are  very 
Swedish,  it  seems,  and  the  whole  thing  attempts  pathos,  but  ends  up  with 
bathos. 

K.  U.  Blundered  through  the  whole  thing,  and  calls  it  a  tonal  and  poetical 
blunder;  rather  rhetorical  in  spots,  but  again  positively  full  of  humor;  some- 
thing "citified"  about  it,  too  conscious,  too  sophistical;  it  is  "speechifying" 
more  than  anything  else  ^  takes  a  lot  of  energy  and  is  full  of  irregularities. 

L.  P.  Rather  dramatic,  but  has  no  meaning;  sounds  like  Gaelic  or  Welsh; 
may  be  recounting  some  tale. 

M.  P.     Sort  of  a  joke;  interesting  and  very  light;  not  hard  to  read;  it  is 


ELEMENTS  OF  SPEECH  IN  RELATION  TO  POETRY        113 

not  important,  but  it  goes  well ;  full  of  spirit,  and  sounds  a  little  condescending 
and  amiably  superior  at  times,  but  she  could  not  take  it  seriously  at  all; 
(laughed  much). 

TENNYSON,  EXPERIMENT  XV 

Transmogrification  of  "Crossing  the  Bar,"  with  the  most 
used  sounds  in  the  poem  repeated  to  fill  out  the  last  few  iambics. 

Thu  won  ing  teev  en  boun  az  pla  nad  kernz 
Too  rawl  net  beme  es  nove  and  barling  meez 
For  nus  when  stam  which  krame  thu  bide  us  fo 
And  tow  but  flon  ing  twi  nes  mob  ro  sleme 
When  fur  ma  le  put  chad  ing  rabe  thu  nark 
For  soo  nay  drel  ter  gam  thu  ho  nad  wi 
Thar  bees  a  tow  from  e  land  tepe  a  dov 
Kla  das  me  thi  for  dav  6  lound  thu  te 
When  taf  ite  bem  too  sud  thar  tow  f  ren  soo 
For  meth  6m  tise  and  po  nad  uv  re  tha. 

INTROSPECTION 

B.  P.  Easy  to  say  and  produces  a  feeling  of  apprehension;  never  got 
exactly  into  it,  and  the  imagery  and  emotions  were  indefinite;  (subject 
pondered  the  passage  quite  a  while  after  the  experiment,  but  no  further 
introspection  was  obtained)  ;  kinaesthesis  is  felt  on  the  lips  mostly. 

F.  N.  Rather  easy  to  say,  and  there  is  a  good  deal  of  openness  about  the 
sounds,  but  no  imagery  came;  feels  often  that  the  unaccented  vowels  ought 
not  to  be  long. 

K.  P.  It  is  very  temperamental,  and  at  times  slightly  melancholy;  thinks 
of  ploughed  ground  and  gets  ever  olfactory  imagery;  but  there  is  also  a 
slight  monotony  (sameness)  about  it,  and  at  the  end  there  came  a  feeling  of 
something  like  listlessness. 

L.  P.  It's  very  nice,  but  does  not  provoke  a  big  reaction;  seems  to  be 
describing  a  sad  and  tragic  event;  probably  the  death  of  a  certain  person; 
thinks  of  many  perils,  enemies,  trepidation  and  the  like.  The  sounds  are 

wonderfully  good. 

M.  P.  Rather  easy  to  say,  and  line  7  is  charming ;  doesn't  seem  very  serious 
and  makes  one  think  of  the  sounds  of  nature;  gets  imagery  of  the  woods, 
fields  and  the  like ;  but  the  whole  effect  is  quite  steady  and  self-contained. 

ARNOLD,  EXPERIMENT  XV 

Transmogrification  of   "Dover  Beach,"   employing  the   first 
eleven  lines  and  a    part  of  the  twelfth. 


ii4  ROBERT  CHENAULT  GIVLER 

Thu  nam  iz  dile  and  stra  land  son  ter  ve 
R6  nare  thu  ton  quil  bine  thu  seng  6v  mern 
Whar  kind  ing  spreem  thu  stal  chen  tife  iz  ko 
Thu  gon  f rin  lis  6v  stam  blez  nare  thu  chee 
Tu  flis  thu  whee  nad  zil  thu  hur  dum  then 
Thu  se  dran  wape  eu'towm  at  stin  6v  noom 
Thu  ti  f  rol  wete  thu  bafe  up  tole  thu  gris 
Chi  pang  thu  stabe  ing  krand  hi  thai  thu  gmd 
Tu  stoon  thu  traf  iz  glake  II  kos  be  sweemz 
Blan  or  hlz  roon  daw  nole  thu  gli  men  zave. 

INTROSPECTION 

B.  U.  Traces  of  pleasantness  in  the  first  part,  but  at  the  end  it  was 
rather  sarcastic,  i.e.  full  of  a  sort  of  "Schadenfreude."  Too  many  "s"  and  "z" 
sounds  in  it,  and  too  many  unusual  sound  combinations ;  the  first  six  lines 
are  better  poetical  constructions  than  the  last  four;  tried  to  like  it,  but 
the  kinaesthetic  factor  dominated. 

F.  U.  The  first  line  is  not  so  bad,  but  the  rest  are  horrible,  and  he  does 
not  think  there  can  be  any  such  sounds  in  poetry;  a  lot  of  the  words  give 
him  pains  in  the  face,  such  as  "glake,"  "gind,"  "gris,"  etc. 

K.  P.  Got  a  very  distinct  feeling  of  standing  up  and  "giving  it  to 
some  one"  in  a  rhetorical  manner;  there  is  a  great  deal  of  reserve  strength 
in  it,  and  the  pronunciation  is  very  prominent;  not  moody,  like  some  of  the 
others,  but  rather  stern  and  a  trifle  polemical. 

L.  P.  It's  tragic ;  thinks  of  a  combat ;  there  seems  to  be  something  dramatic, 
moving  and  forceful  about  it;  visualizes  a  storm  at  sea,  through  which  the 
vessel  finally  rode  to  safety.  This  was  due  to  the  associational  element  in 
the  sound  themselves. 

M.  P.  At  first  it  was  very  heavy  and  labored,  and  did  not  delight  her  soul ; 
then  it  became  better,  and  visual  imagery  of  the  sea  with  people  talking  in  a 
dignified  and  probably  hushed  manner  about  it. 

SHEKESPEARE,  EXPERIMENT  XV 
Transmogrification  of  the  LXIVth  sonnet,  lines  I  to  10. 

Thu  rene  hov  ki  when  fost  hav  bimz  el  taje 
De  rand  orn  te  prid  laste  an  ser  boud  mowrz 
Whi  tazed  it  ras  woun  slaje  al  chis  and  vore 
Ne  bri  too  sof  dal  mos  thu  tow  gri  hav 
Fen  ger  tav  so  mi  heen  ad  shan  ij  ta 
Ti  kos  un  woil  thu  tos  6m  ding  thu  wand 
Hov  stind  ing  kov  erm  ane  thu  Ion  ter  krees 


ELEMENTS  OF  SPEECH  IN  RELATION  TO  POETRY        115 

Whin  or  thu  stun  ith  cheem  and  nis  fon  or 
De  fanj  when  too  with  sta  kav  i  wit  stoun 
With  ta  chus  or  shed  melf  or  16s  ter  na. 

INTROSPECTION 

B.  P.  Has  much  emotional  value,  and  there  seems  to  be  a  sexual  element 
running  through  it;  but  very  rich  and  refined,  even  if  voluptuous,  and  it  might 
be  taken  from  a  poem  which  contains  a  sex-philosophy  of  life;  the  lip 
sounds  and  the  "j"  and  "ch"  sounds  are  particularly  predominant. 

F.  P.  Means  nothing  to  him  and  the  pleasure  is  in  the  rhythm  only,  "j,' 
"ch,"  and  "sh'  sounds  quite  prominent. 

K.  P.  Seems  to  be  of  low  pitch  and  is  slightly  provocative  of  melancholy; 
visualizes  a  market-place  full  of  people;  the  mood  is  not  depressing,  but  the 
kind  of  a  melancholy  that  one  takes  delight  in.  "De  rand  orn'  is  very 
fine  tonally. 

L.  P.  Very  nice  and  smooth;  narrative  and  not  dramatic;  is  like  Shakes- 
peare in  Othello  where  the  story  of  the  ships  being  lost  is  narrated  (  ?  query, 
Merchant  of  Venice).  Got  no  imagery,  but  tried  to. 

M.  U.  Very  doleful  and  depressing;  recalls  the  "Flying  Dutchman,"  and 
all  the  attendant  weirdness  of  it;  it  is  minor  music  all  through;  at  the  second 
reading,  it  got  insistently  pathetic  as  in  describing  a  great  loss  that  was 
irreparable. 

WORDSWORTH,  EXPERIMENT  XV 

Transmogrification  of  the  opening  lines  of  the  "Ode  on  the 
Intimations  of  Immortality."  Employing  the  first  fifty  accented 
and  unaccented  sounds. 

Thu  stree  bel  grame  it  vo  nes  mer  nad  le 
When  gli  nad  vare  chul  par  mon  thel  n  dem 
Too  mm  did  nore  pas  tmgz  ern  dree  wo  nese 
I  tro  verz  kom  thu  shemz  6  wen  duth  ra 
De  nowd  6v  kom  I  tese  thu  ma  li  zore 
Whi  na  chand  thi  luz  f  rem  her  tow  kri  sare 
Ath  yome  thu  ris  hav  zee  naz  dow  thu  tine 
Or  whinz  u  tabe  so  beth  6t  hi  nad  wit 
Kee  var  noot  whi  thu  zen  ad  sil  bo  mth 
De  goze  ar  vul  han  dize  thu  noom  a  nar. 

INTROSPECTION 

B.  N.  Seems  rather  matter  of  fact;  not  what  he  calls  poetry,  because  it  is 
rather  narrative  and  epical,  not  lyric  and  free;  seemed  easy  enough  to  learn 


n6  ROBERT  CHENAULT  CILLER 

to  say  it,  and  the  kinaesthesis  is  forward;  the  "z,"  "s,"  "d,"  "th,"  and  "t"  sound 
prominent. 

F.  P.  Rather  hard  to  say;  calls  it  "Kammersprache,"  rather  than  poetry; 
the  sensations  aroused  are  akin  to  those  experienced  while  walking  over  a 
muddy,  frozen  ground.  No  imagery. 

K.  P.  (Arouses  melancholy  at  once ;  visualizes  an  English  moor,  over  which 
he  seems  to  be  walking;  obscure  feeling  all  the  way  through,  as  if  hesi- 
tating to  say  or  do  something;  the  prominent  sound  is  the  "wh." 

L.  P.  Only  a  very  slight  organic  quiver  aroused  by  the  poem ;  it  is  barbaric, 
but  enjoyable;  thought  of  Norsemen  by  association,  and  also  of  the  Goths. 
The  feelings  are  rather  lukewarm. 

M.  P.  Images  a  cliff  overlooking  the  sea  and  of  someone  on  the  cliff  telling 
tales  of  the  sea;  it  is  very  appealing  and  peculiar;  thought  there  were  many 
full  cadences  in  the  poem.  At  times  it  became  very  confidential. 

SHELLEY,  EXPERIMENT  XV 

Transmogrification  of  the  first  fifty  accented  and  unaccented 
syllables  of  the  "Stanzas  written  in  dejection,  near  Naples." 

Thu  marn  is  wering  klave  un  wile  ed  zare 
With  sant  enz  brem  thu  tound  it  par  thu  sko 
A  win  iz  noom  eld  voin  i  sul  its  bree 
Thu  nelf  iz  per  shun  zit  ent  rast  thu  vound. 
Tri  16  brand  shon  I  pand  thu  gre  lov  serth 
De  moidz  it  nus  thu  swen  lex  merdz  up  tise 
And  stee  kar  soth  I  bmdz  H  tois  thu  do  ft 
Zan  eeps  Tz  pern  ik  fees  up  stron  thu  trove 
Ze  pam  thu  floodz  wi  bors  pul  ti  wud  lis 
Thu  wuz  6v  tos  I  zud  lu  flaves  ed  wi. 

INTROSPECTION 

B.  P.  Gets  imagery  of  the  sea  and  the  splash  of  the  surf  at  once;  seems 
very  onomatopoetic ;  and  in  spite  of  the  fact  that  there  is  much  "s"  and  "z" 
in  it,  it  is  pleasant,  but  by  virtue  of  the  imagery  only;  gives  a  vague  feeling 
of  uneasiness  and  there  are  many  bodily  tensions. 

F.  P.  Rather  moody  and  sombre  feeling  aroused  by  it ;  there  is  much  mouth 
movement  and  one  has  to  slide  to  some  of  the  words  and  stop  hesitatingly 
before  others  of  them ;  feels  tense,  not  on  account  of  the  pronunciation  wholly, 
but  on  account  of  the  mood. 

K.  P.  Feels  as  if  he  is  reading  a  somewhat  morbid  fairy  story,  and  the 
emotion  is  one  of  mystery  and  helplessness;  this  comes  direct,  and  is  not 
associational  at  all ;  at  first  there  was  a  feeling  of  withdrawing  from  the 
mood,  but  this  soon  ceased ;  it  seems  to  be  bound  up  in  the  sounds  themselves 


ELEMENTS  OF  SPEECH  IN  RELATION  TO  POETRY        n? 

and  in  nothing  else;  the  frequency  of  the  long  vowels  being  unaccented  seems 
to  heighten  the  power  of  the  poem  to  sustain  its  mood. 

L.  N.  The  sounds  are  good,  but  he  cannot  feel  the  connection  between 
them;  now  and  then  it  seemed  as  if  the  whole  thing  would  get  unified, 
but  it  never  did. 

M.  U.  Very  depressing  and  disappointing;  arouses  a  mood  in  which  one 
feels  helpless;  so  many  interesting  sounds  in  it,  like  "gree  lov  serth,"  etc. 
The  first  five  lines  are  cheerful  enough,  but  the  last  five  are  doleful. 

COWPER,  EXPERIMENT  XV 

Transmogrification  of  the  well  known  lines:  "Knowledge 
and  wisdom,  far  from  being  one,"  etc.  (This  was  selected,  not 
as  poetry,  especially,  but  as  a  test  of  whether  the  transmogrifi- 
cation of  a  didactic,  homiletic  poem  would  be  successful. ) 

Hav  nol  ing  bar  dom  fon  ij  ween  6m  elz 
And  61  f  ron  wek  ith  noft  er  mz  re  dwen 
Km  plo  dij  mawts  6v  theet  wun  huth  imz  or 
Shorn  wen  tin  miz  that  rone  tiv  zede  ma  tmds 
Thu  pras  ij  mof  ich  too  nad  zome  its  bood 
A  were  alz  tase  Tl  smared  un  tlz  it  plere 
And  with  6m  dooth  whar  zild  en  fol  ble  chim 
Diz  boo  that  rowd  no  prit  us  kernd  6m  sque 
So  lut  haz  wheemz  it  chumber  nat  iz  hoom 
He  sumble  niz  ded  wuth  en  hum  diz  nol. 

INTROSPECTION 

B.  P.  The  prominent  sound  elements  are  the  "n-drone,"  the  dentals  and  the 
labials;  got  no  imagery  and  no  meaning;  commonplace. 

F.  P.  Very  queer  thing;  full  of  pauses,  and  the  rhythm  feels  like  the 
different  steps  in  a  fancy  dance ;  besides  the  rhythm,  there  is  not  much  to  it ; 
as  far  as  meaning  is  concerned,  it  sounds  like  optimistic  speech-making. 

K.  U.  Seems  cool,  emotionally;  rather  rhetorical  and  arouses  no  imagery; 
in  spite  of  its  poverty  of  emotion,  and  its  unpleasantness,  it  is  interesting. 

L.  N.    It  is  not  dramatical,  deep,  or  poetical;  it's  like  Pope. 

M.  P.  Amusing;  like  some  moral  story  to  be  told  to  youngsters;  line  i 
starts  out  grand  and  almost  epical,  and  then  the  whole  thing  tumbles  and 
never  regains  itself  till  the  end. 

DRYDEN,  EXPERIMENT  XV 

Transmogrification  of  lines  94  to  103  inclusive,  of  "Absalom 
and  Achitophel." 


n8  ROBERT  CHENAULT  GIVLER 

De  mov  er  prand  6m  dall  isht  az  him  pived 
And  lub  ez  tesh  ar  fland  6v  lood  uz  blard 
Ka  host  wer  yax  and  gret  thar  wand  eld  bodz 
Too  gernt  on  stom  this  ka  thike  sood  thar  whame 
Dal  prot  66d  in  then  flet  thu  he  mis  prall 
And  sar  thu  stee  thunz  jil  6v  ma  for  stees 
Re  bod  so  rese  thar  hent  6v  hiz  de  gome 
Li  gruth  or  whee  bood  pone  6k  stenz  in  sar 
Haz  dole  thu  f  eeb  en  gat  his  orn  stin  teeb 
Hov  ed  ants  if  he  had  iz  ber  va  dole. 

INTROSPECTION 

B.  P.  Predominantly  dental;  seemed  rather  easy  to  learn;  quite  matter  of 
fact  and  denotes  activity  of  a  non-poetic  character;  some  people  are  doing 
something, — all  the  way  from  arguing  to  moving  vigorously  about. 

F.  N.  Very  unpleasant  caesural  pauses ;  very  heavy  and  clumsy,  and  cannot 
be  lyric  poetry,  nor  written  by  any  one  who  knew  the  musical  value  of  sounds; 
tried  to  get  away  from  it  and  couldn't. 

K.  P.  (Laughed.)  Humorous  and  countrified;  seems  to  be  telling  some 
"yarn," — a  good  big  one;  the  speaker  is  perfectly  willing  to  hear  himself 
talk;  tonally,  it  is  just  an  interesting  collection  of  noises. 

L.  N.  Got  no  reaction  from  it  at  all;  it  never  drew  his  attention  fully 
and  he  began  to  think  of  other  things. 

M.  U.  Feels  like  going  over  a  rocky  road ;  felt  no  rhythmical  swing  except 
at  "hiz  de  gome";  no  imagery. 

DRYDEN,  EXPERIMENT  XVI 
Transmogrification  of  lines  66  to  78  of  "Alexander's  Feast." 

Thu  vound  ith  hane  hiz  dooth  and  or  gra  sail 
F66  nabe  thu  gmg  ed  slantelz  tase  he  koze 
And  thrasmg  foo  thu  rail  iz  towness  rad 
Thu  mize  and  slawment  gloter  chize  thu  fev 
Awt  thride  hiz  kees  en  sar  we  hile  diz  ther 
Hen  f  rise  en  prand  hiz  chorn  iz  mew  he  danj 
And  tek  is  chi  hin  dall  6ft  wheer  ful  zome 
He  tewz  en  chall  e  zood  and  fise  he  tafe 
Di  hon  hiz  rit  and  ta  de  soof  ha  grail 
En  pife  us  ta  se  fung  ba  stige  a  toove. 

INTROSPECTION 

B.  P.  (?)  Hard  to  say;  the  "s*  and  "z"  sounds  predominate,  and  somehow 
make  it  sound  gummy ;  feels  that  the  cheeks  have  moved  a  great  deal ;  hissing 


ELEMENTS  OF  SPEECH  IN  RELATION  TO  POETRY        ug 

and  breathing  is  frequent;  it  is  both  difficult  and  amusing,  and  he  cannot  get 
any  meaning  out  of  it. 

F.  P.  Thinks  of  trotting  horses  and  movement  in  the  open  air;  but  this 
he  holds  to  be  due  to  a  direct  association  from  the  words  "soof"  (=  hoof?) 
and  "ritand"  (=  reiten  [Ger.]  which  he  pronounced  with  a  long  "I"). 
Goes  easily  and  quickly. 

K.  P.  Very  smooth  and  interesting;  gets  a  cobwebby  mood  of  mystery, 
but  doesn't  know  why;  feels  that  many  of  the  expressions  in  such  a  passage 
will  turn  into  words,  if  one  looks  for  words. 

L.  N.  Gets  no  reaction  whatever.  (N.B.^Between  this  and  the  previous 
passage  experimented  upon,  Subject  L.  relieved  his  mind  of  certain  matters 
which  were  annoying  him,  but  not  even  then  did  any  reaction  to  the  above 
poem  take  place.) 

M.  U.  Dislikes  the  looks  of  it ;  it  sounds  blatant  and  impudent  and  is  full 
of  the  most  difficult  combinations  possible. 

SPENSER,  EXPERIMENT  XV 

Transmogrification  of  Stanza  34,  Canto  I,  "The  Faery 
Queene,"  with  the  most  used  sounds  repeated  to  complete  the  last 
line. 

In  laz  it  wertle  ho  mi  lit  a  taje 

Li  fide  a  sown  as  dard  6v  or  est  ede 

Re  hale  id  zar  dom  torple  tath  al  fas 

El  pav  a  til  bi  frotle  woo  thel  dite 

Thu  trotle  fide  ar  win  li  haz  i  cher 

Too  mingz  it  hade  li  sont  ar  tew  f  ren  de 

And  bide  li  porn  hiz  kra  cha  wis  tal  paj 

That  weem  id  ho  den  lit  a  fom  ed  pli 

Chi  thor  li  f  rent  wha  tha  kre  toun  li  wase 

Wha  16  mi  per  nad  fev  ed  strel  in  taje. 

INTROSPECTION 

B.  P.  The  vowels  seems  very  predominant;  the  dental  consonants  rather 
numerous  also;  very  poetical  substance  in  it,  but  cannot  get  at  it;  upon  a 
second  reading  notices  the  liquids  more  than  the  dentals,  and  the  sounds 
seem  very  open,  but  this  does  not  make  it  at  all  oratorical, — rather  quiet  and 
restful,  instead. 

F.  P.  The  lines  containing  the  words  that  end  with  "tie"  remind  him  of 
Maeterlinck's  "Blue  Bird" ;  direct  association ;  some  of  it  very  easy  to  say 
and  some  of  it  very  hard;  wonders  what  the  frequent  repetition  of  the 
word  "taje"  means. 

K.  P.  Easy  to  say;  not  exactly  melancholy,  but  something  very  akin  to  it; 
no  content  suggested,  just  this  strange  feeling  of  artistic  melancholy. 

L.    P.     Excellent   Jabberwocky;    got   a    rippling    feeling   down    the   back; 


120  ROBERT  CHENAULT  GIVLER 

doesn't  seem  to  be  anything  very  tragic  and  vital;  just  like  some  nice  little 
quiet  talk. 

M.  P.  The  words  ending  in  "tie"  are  at  first  very  quieting;  then  the 
"look"  of  the  letter  "j"  annoys  and  seems  to  color  the  whole  thing;  would 
become  unpleasant  upon  very  slight  provocation. 

SHAKESPEARE,  EXPERIMENT  XIII 

Shall  I  believe 

That  unsubstantial  death  is  amorous, 
And  that  the  lean  abhorred  monster  keeps 
Thee  here  in  dark  to  be  his  paramour? 
For  fear  of  that,  I  still  will  stay  with  thee: 
And  never  from  this  palace  of  dim  night 
Depart   again:   here,   here,   will   I   remain 
With  worms  that  are  thy  chambermaids ;  O  here 
Will  I  set  up  my  everlasting  rest, 
And  shake  the  yoke  of  inauspicious  stars 
From  this  world-wearied  flesh. 

SHAKESPEARE,  EXPERIMENT  XIV 
Thu  Ian  sal  de  sher  nan  at  rus  ab  sten 
Iz  km  ul  meeps  and  han  be  m.6r  sub  lith 
In  theer  sha  dal  hiz  par  thed  both  il  stoor 
And  eep  thov  hane  too  stal  f  er  tov  ith  reesp 
Sti  fev  or  nom  is  gee  ful  wen  de  thite 
Wer  pern  das  hane  a  chi  we  la  wil  freer 
Bo  ma  ther  zud  im  spar  that  hi  with  pame 
Re  sher  ing  tev  et  rast  thu  ka  weer  flun 
Mi  les  and  weer  6v  ko  ther  yast  id  reesh 
Hi  zar  mold  sith  aw  rik  wer  zim  us  reet. 

Shakespeare,  Experiment  XIV.  Transmogrification  of  XIII 

INTROSPECTION 

B.  P.  The  sibilants  do  not  disturb,  although  they  are  very  numerous;  gets 
visual  imagery  of  the  woods  and  the  sea;  the  general  aspect  is  quiet  and 
solemn ;  seems  restrained  and  hushed ;  no  activity  in  the  notion  aroused, — can 
hardly  tell  what  it  is. 

F.  P.  Seems  delicate  and  soft,  with  only  a  few  interruptions  such  as 
"reesp" ;  rhythm  is  both  quickened  and  slowed  in  places,  and  he  rather  likes 
the  necessity  to  stop  and  begin  again  at  a  different  tempo;  feels  like  the 
resolution  of  dissonances,  every  time  it  occurs,  which  is  usually  after  a 
difficult  word,  or  one  that  causes  readjustment  of  the  vocal  organs  afterwards. 


ELEMENTS  OF  SPEECH  IN  RELATION  TO  POETRY        121 

K.  P.  Smooth  and  easy  to  say;  doesn't  get  any  definite  imagery,  but 
the  general  effect  produced  is  rather  subdued;  thinks  of  either  a  calm  on 
the  sea,  or  a  suspense  of  activity;  the  thing  has  a  lot  of  meaning,  but  it  is 
very  subtle,  and  for  him,  latent. 

L.  P.  Not  epic,  but  lyric;  seems  pastoral,  rather  than  anything  else; 
the  opening  lines  reminded  him  of  the  tonal  effect  of  Gray's  Elegy. 

M.  P.  Would  not  have  been  surprised  to  have  heard  an  organ  keep  up  the 
tonal  effect  after  the  end  of  the  passage  came;  there  is  a  rumble  of  heavy, 
grand  tones  underneath,  as  it  were,  the  sounds  as  spoken;  it  is  not  the 
rhythm  that  is  the  prominent  feature,  but  the  sound-mass,  which  is  surpris- 
ingly new  and  agreeable. 

We  have  omitted  from  this  list  a  small  number  of  experiments 
made  after  the  same  pattern :  Coleridge's  "Christabel"  was  tried, 
but  proved  introspectively  unsuccessful;  likewise  three  songs  from 
Shakespeare  made  over  into  five-line  passages, — "Hark,  hark, 
the  lark,"  Ariel's  Song,  and  the  Boy's  Song  from  "Measure  for 
Measure."  Likewise  two  passages  from  Swinburne's  "Laus 
Veneris,"  two  from  Rossetti's  "Blessed  Damosel,"  and  one  of 
Sydney's  Sonnets.  Jonson's  "Drink  to  me  only  with  thine  eyes" 
as  well  as  a  passage  from  Pope's  "Essay  on  Man"  fell  flat. 

The  writer  usually  found  it  more  difficult  to  transmogrify 
the  shorter  verse  forms  into  decasyllabic  lines  than  the  others. 
Tonal  replicas  were  less  easily  elicited  from  such  passages,  which 
having  been  cast  into  a  form  tonally  demanding  other  than  the 
decasyllabic  pattern,  remained  recalcitrant  to  the  pulverizing 
and  agglutinizing  process  of  this  experimental  method.  Soft  as 
the  tonal  data  of  poetry  may  be,  yet  it  would  appear  that  the 
various  form-orders  of  verse  lie  not  in  intersecting  series. 

RANK  LISTS  FOR  THE  EXPERIMENTS   PERFORMED  DURING  THE  THIRD  YEAR'S 

WORK 


> 

> 

> 

^ 
X 

X 

;> 

X 

^- 
X 

K^- 

X 

X 

X 

X 

E 

> 

E 

£ 

j= 

u     >• 

X      X 

X 

u 

M 

•a 

be 
_c 

o 

en 

rt 
B 

a, 

in 

X 

2 

rt 

B 

0. 
en 

rt 
a, 

in 

o 

in 

rt       K> 
CU        rS 
O, 
01         >v 

.2       c 

B 

B 

9 

(u    jy 

I.     Mean 

rt 

8 

o 

C 

C 

rt 

o 

c 

M 

rt 

rt 

h 

0 

rt      "3 

Subject 

W       PQ 

O 

5 

t-l 

cu 

H 

CO 

^ 

CO 

.C 

in 

CO       CO 

B    

c        c 

d 

e 

c 

d 

c 

c 

b 

b 

c 

c       e 

F  

e        e 

c 

d 

e 

e 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d 

d       c 

K    

a  (throughout) 

L        

b       b       h       h 

b 

b 

b 

b 

c 

c 

b 

b        b 

M. 

d       d 

e 

c 

d 

c 

C 

e 

e 

e 

e 

e        d 

122  ROBERT  CHENAULT  GIVLER 


X 


X 

X 

c 

> 
X 

i—  i 
X 

X 

X 

X 

o 
c 

X 

X      ^ 

> 

ir^ 
V         O 
c3         eg 

*• 

3 

•.n 

'13 

c 

e 

3 

CO 

^"»           r* 

X 

—      £. 

C/)             VI 

a 

1 

1 

U 

CO 

*o 

CD 

•a 

Ja 
.S 

i 

V 

0          ° 

•§      S 

CO 

ex 

en       ri 

6 

0 

CO 

o 

o 

p 

1 

co 

_ex 

>••       o 
CO        ft 

o 
PL, 

CO      V3 

B.   , 

c 

b 

c 

c 

d 

C 

d 

C 

c       e 

d 

c       c 

F  

d 

d 

e 

d 

c 

d 

c 

d 

d       c 

e 

e        c 

K  

.a 

(throughout) 

L  

b 

c 

b 

b 

b 

b 

b 

b 

b       b 

b 

b       b 

M  

16 

e 

d 

e 

e 

e 

e 

e 

e       d 

c 

d        e 

i—  i 
X 

1—1      !> 

x    x 

X 

»—  i 
X 

X 

> 

> 

X 

<u 
bo 

X 

bo 

.s 

X 
o 

u 

!_ 

cO 
CO 
tt 

X 

<U            Ct> 

l-l         l-l 

CO           CO 
CO             CO 

ex      ex 

je 

t^ 
o 

& 

"     > 

S    x 

ex 

II.    M.V. 
Subject 

0> 

ri 

co 

W 

o 
pq 

O 

"E 
<u 

3 

'5 
£ 

o 

pq 

>> 

u 

H 

en 
U 

.M 

CO 
JS 

CO 

*o 
C 

en           en 
CO           CO 
J«J         .* 

co          cO 

jn       JT 
co      co 

en 
"O 

O 
£ 

o          >. 

£      § 

cO          co 
£        $ 

B  

b 

b 

a 

a 

a 

d 

c 

d 

e        a 

a 

e       b 

F  , 

.a 

e 

b 

d 

e 

b 

d 

c 

b        e 

c 

a       a 

K  

,e 

a 

c 

b 

d 

c 

b 

a 

d        c 

b 

d       d 

L  

d 

c 

d 

c 

c 

a 

a 

b 

c       b 

d 

c        e 

M  

c 

d 

e 

e 

b 

e 

e 

e 

a        d 

e 

b       c 

X 

i—i 

M 

>—  i 
X 

&     G 

rs        X 

4J              CO 

In 

CD 

c 

X 

X 

X 
c 

X 

c 

u 

c 

3 

X 
i-i 

CO 

X      £ 
ir""       c 

X 

t-i              U 
V            CO 

ex       o< 

O, 
> 

"c 

4J 

co 

•a 

"O 

.a 

c 

i 

n       I 

CO 

CO            'L 

•f- 
O 

'5 

CO 

en 
O 

09 

O 

c< 

C 

P 

l-l 

p 

'S 
CO 

u 
ex 
/) 

•o       c 
c^      £ 

ex 
o 
PL, 

rt         co 
CO        CO 

B.   . 

b 

e 

c 

e 

a 

d 

c 

e 

b       c 

b 

c       b 

F.    . 

.a 

c 

a 

a 

b 

c 

b 

b 

c       a 

c 

b       d 

K.  . 

a 

e 

c 

e 

b 

d 

c 

a       d 

e 

e       e 

L  

.d 

b 

d 

b 

d 

a 

a 

a 

e       b 

d 

d       a 

M  

.c 

d 

b 

d 

c 

e 

e 

d 

d       e 

a 

a       c 

RANK  LISTS 

FOR  THE 

EXPERIMENTS 

PERFORMED 

DURING  THE 

t—  i 

THIRD 

YEAR 

> 

X 

H-  1 
I—I             ^ 
t> 

x    x 

X 

X 

X 

X 

X 

i-, 

...3. 

CO           CO 

•£ 

£     > 

X 

> 

X 

> 

w 

bo 

bo 
.S 

c 
o 

cO 

<u 
ex 

X 

CO           CO 
CO            CO 

t-H 

O 

cO       k> 
CO        ?S 

ex 

X 

en 

en 

en           en 

en 

T. 

c 

co       jy 

III.    Rnj. 
Subject 

^ 

<D 

o 

u 

PQ 

>> 

CO 

O 

'o 
U 

O 

pq 

C 

u 

H 

CO 
CO 

o 

E 

tyl                              ^/ 

CO           CO 
JS         M 
CO        CO 

l-l 

O 

co     *o3 
co     co 

B  

.C 

c 

d 

d 

b 

a 

e 

b 

d       a 

b 

c        c 

F  

b 

b 

c 

a 

a 

e 

b 

c 

b       b 

a 

a       b 

K  

.e 

d 

b 

e 

d 

c 

a 

d 

e       d 

e 

e        e 

L  

.a 

a 

a 

c 

c 

b 

c 

a 

a       c 

c 

b       a 

M  

d 

e 

e 

b 

e 

d 

d 

e 

c        e 

d 

d       d 

ELEMENTS  OF  SPEECH  IN  RELATION  TO  POETRY        123 


>*>>>>*> 

X       S      X      X       X      X        c       * 


X 


X 


B  

<u 
fit 

0 

U 
d 

">  Swinbui 

0  Rossetti 

i"  Rossetti 

u 

•o 

I* 

Q 
d 

<y 

Q 
a 

n>  Swinbui 

O) 

§ 

(U 

O4 

Cfl 

C 

V 

'O 

to 
c 

c 
o 

CO 

Pi 
O 

TT 

X 

u 

o. 
o 

e 

"  Shakesp 

CD 

in 
c 

F  

a 

a 

b 

a 

c 

d 

b 

d 

a 

b 

h 

K  

e 

d 

e 

c 

b 

d 

a 

d 

e 

c 

d 

e 

e 

L  

a 

h 

b 

e 

c 

b 

c 

a 

h 

b 

c 

a 

a 

M. 

.  .c 

e 

d 

d 

e 

e 

b 

e 

a 

e 

b 

d 

d 

The  rank  lists  for  these  experiments  show  much  steadiness, 
but  inasmuch  as  there  were  but  five  subjects,  the  possible  devia- 
tion from  position  is  very  slight.  Correlation  between  the  feeling 
tone  and  the  motor  discharge  remains  the  same  as  it  was  in  the 
second  year's  work;  to  the  very  end,  the  mean  variation  and  the 
range  do  not  seem  to  have  anything  to  do  with  the  matter,  or  at 
least,  very  little  in  comparison  with  the  mean  of  the  tappings. 

Let  us  turn  to  the  graphs  for  these  26  experiments.  In  nearly 
every  case  the  "short"  vowels,  whether  accented  or  unaccented, 
and  the  explosive  consonants  produce  a  greater  motor  discharge 
than  the  softer,  "slower"  and  more  mellifluous  sounds  of  the 
language.  We  give  the  figures  for  verification. 


Accented 

Unacc 

Experiment 

short  vowels 

short  A 

Keats  XV  

..      25 

39 

Byron    XV  

..      24 

33 

Arnold  XV  

19 

39 

Tennyson  XV  

12 

35 

Shakespeare  XVI  .  .  . 

10 

16 

Shakespeare  XVII.. 

8 

18 

Shakespeare  XVIII. 

IO 

19 

Cowper  XV  

..     27 

40 

Shelley  XV  

18 

42 

Wordsworth   XV... 

..       18 

30 

Gray  XV.  . 

14 

39 

Sydney  XV  

14 

28 

Jonson  XV  

21 

3i 

Pope  XV  

••         23 

40 

Accented 
explosive 
consonants 

Unaccented 
explosive 
consonants 

43 
IS 

15 

24 

27 
36 

18 

24 

15 
17 
18 

ii 

5 
8 

33 
34 

18 

23 
18 
20 

32 
38 

20 

21 

28 
29 

28 

18 

I24  ROBERT  CHENAULT  GIVLER 

Accented        Unaccented 

Accented         Unaccented       explosive          explosive 
Experiment  short  vowels    short  vowels    consonants       consonants 

Spenser  XV 18  4*  43  19 

Browning  XV 24  32  31  20 

Dryden   XV 25  33  4O  19 

Swinburne  XV 19  31  3i  IS 

Dryden   XVI 10  38  38  17 

Swinburne   XVI 15  27  24  18 

Rossetti  XVI 26  27  37  13 

Shakespeare  XV 19  28  24  21 

Rossetti  XV 19  33  27  17 

By  referring  to  the  graphing  for  the  two  experiments  on 
Shakespeare,  XIII  and  XIV,  it  is  found  again  in  this  case,  as  we 
have  noticed  before,  that  the  transmogrification  of  a  passage  of 
poetry  tends  to  arouse  the  motor  consciousness  more  than  does 
the  original  poem. 

From  the  above  results,  it  seems  clear  that  the  short  vowels 
and  the  explosive  consonants,  regardless  of  accented  or  unac- 
cented position  in  the  poetic  foot  tend  to  produce  the  strong 
motor  arousals ;  but  this  was  not  the  case  with  the  earlier  experi- 
ments in  which  the  single  line  was  repeated  five  times  in  succes- 
sion; nevertheless,  the  summation  of  effects  is  evidently  what 
accounts  for  it,  together  with  other  factors  not  to  be  overlooked. 
The  motor  setting  preparatory  to  tapping  a  long  passage  of  verse 
is  different  from  the  motor  setting  which  merely,  repeats  the 
same  line  over  and  over  again ;  and  with  the  appearance  of  new 
combinations  a  stronger  effect  is  produced  by  the  addition  of  like 
elements  than  by  a  great  variety  of  elements  giving  no  effect  of 
homogeneity ;  once  the  feeling  produced  by  the  short  vowels  and 
explosive  consonants  is  aroused,  even  the  lessening  of  their  num- 
ber per  line  in  the  following  lines  might  not  show  as  soon  in  the 
motor  consciousness  as  it  did  in  the  introspective  consciousness ; 
instances  of  this  we  have  seen  in  the  previous  pages.  But  it  does 
not  seem  to  work  the  other  way  around, — the  effect  of  explosive 
sounds  is  immediate  upon  the  motor  consciousness,  and  one  such 
sound  can  mar  the  effect  of  an  otherwise  placid  and  liquid  line, 
and  this  may  account  for  the  apparent  partial  lack  of  definite 


ELEMENTS  OF  SPEECH  IN  RELATION  TO  POETRY        125 

one  to  one  correspondence  which  we  have  sought  for  in  connec- 
tion with  our  study  of  the  motor  energies  and  the  introspective 
consciousness  both  singly  and  together. 

Allied  to  the  characteristic  form-quality  in  the  graphs  for 
each  individual  poet,  especially  in  the  more  meaningful  lines 
experimented  upon,  is  the  matter  of  the  tapped  strokes  as  they 
appeared  upon  the  smoked  paper  ribbon.  After  they  had  become 
accustomed  to  the  tapping,  every  one  of  the  subjects  tapped  in 
what  could  be  called  a  thoroughly  individual  manner.  Some 
of  them  tapped  slowly  and  with  great  deliberation,  thereby  mak- 
ing a  visible  record  of  very  rounded  loops;  others  would  react 
by  a  very  quick  down-stroke,  followed  by  a  slow,  hesitating 
up-stroke,  while  still  others  would  tap  strokes  that  appeared  on 
the  paper  as  very  fine  points,  or  even  in  some  cases  would  move 
the  finger  so  quickly  that  the  pointer  climbed  the  roller  on 
the  up-stroke  and  returned  with  sudden  relaxation  of  the  rubber 
band  in  such  a  way  as  to  make  a  loop  in  the  smoky  surface 
of  the  ribbon.  >,And  here  lies  the  interesting  point:  that  in  the 
variously  individual  records  there  appeared  evidence  of  all  felt 
and  unfelt  changes  in  the  emotional  character  of  the  experiments 
presented;  tenseness  of  the  vocal  apparatus  as  well  as  the  op- 
posite state  could  be  told  by  the  experimenter  as  well  as  by 
the  subject,  together  with  subliminal  effects  of  one  sort  or 
another  which  the  subject  did  not  :feel  either  in  summation  or 
otherwise.  Illusions,  also,  of  various  character  were  there 
evidenced,  such  as  temporal  and  numerical  ones.  In  general, 
the  qualitative  and  quantitative  aspects  of  the  visible  record 
amply  supplemented  the  introspection  in  every  way. 

We  have  made  no  special  mention  of  the  time  element  in  con- 
nection with  most  of  these  experiments.  This  is  because  the 
graphs  are  so  typical  for  each  and  all  of  the  subjects,  that  indi- 
vidual mention  is  unnecessary;  furthermore,  the  time  element 
does  not  seem  to  play  any  very  important  role.  It  certainly  is  no 
special  correlate  of  any  of  the  affective  elements  in  consciousness ; 
and  it  does  not  seem  to  be  a  manifest  index  either  of  difficulty 
in  the  material  to  be  recited  or  of  the  number  of  sounds  in  the 
decasyllabic  line.  The  subjects  were  all  told  to  take  their  own 


126  ROBERT  CHENAULT  GIVLER 

time  in  the  tapping;  this  was  merely  to  assure  them  that  they 
were  not  to  be  hurried  in  what  they  did.  This,  however,  is  to 
be  noted  as  the  regular  temporal  manifestation  of  all  the  subjects : 
the  repetition  of  the  same  iambic  line  five  times  usually  showed 
on  the  record  as  having  taken  longer  time  with  each  repetition, — 
that  is,  the  oftener  repeated,  the  slower  it  became,  though  none  of 
the  subjects  were  aware  of  it.  This  may  have  been  due  to  a  num- 
of  things :  either  slight  muscular  fatigue,  or  else  to  the  fact  that 
as  the  impression  aroused  the  introspective  and  the  esthetic  con- 
sciousnesses more  and  more,  less  and  less  nervous  energy  was 
sent  per  impulse  per  unit  of  time  into  the  finger.  That  it  was  not 
due  to  imperfections  in  the  machinery  is  clearly  shown  by  the 
fact  that  the  ribbon  was  allowed  to  pass  several  inches  before  the 
pointer  was  dropped  upon  it  and  the  signal  to  begin  was  given. 

A  very  pertinent  question  to  be  asked  about  all  this  work  is, — 
"What  had  the  subject's  general  condition,  mental  and  otherwise, 
to  do  with  the  results  of  the  experiment?"  A  careful  record  was 
kept  all  during  the  second  and  third  year  as  to  how  the  subject 
felt  at  the  beginning  of  the  experiment  and  the  results  showed 
that  the  main  effects  of  fatigue  and  other  sub-normal  states  were 
of  several  kinds:  i.  A  less  high  degree  of  pleasurability  can  be 
aroused  in  the  state  of  fatigue;  2.  The  mean  variation  of  the 
tappings  on  fatigue  days  is  less  than  on  normal  days,  but  3,  that 
the  subject  did  not  reverse  the  results  of  the  previous  experiments 
at  all,' — those  who  showed  a  positive  correlation  showed  it  still, 
and  those  who  before  had  showed  either  a  definite  negative  corre- 
lation, or  a  scatter  and  miss  correlation  also  continued  to  do  so  ; 
the  more  the  experiment  develops,  the  more  it  seems  that  we 
were  getting  motor  correlations  with  respect  to  the  vocal  appara- 
tus, rather  than  results  which  attached  significance  to  the  total 
psycho-neural  mechanism.  But  to  return  to  the  matter  of  fatigue 
days,  only  one  of  the  subjects,  L.,  tended  ever  to  nullify  his 
previous  results,  but  then  he  also  attempted  to  guess  at  his  own 
type  of  correlation,  and  this  guess  may  have  influenced  the  tap- 
pings for  that  day. 

Not  every  anticipation  or  conjecture  with  which  this  work 


ELEMENTS  OF  SPEECH  IN  RELATION  TO  POETRY        127 

began  has  been  verified  by  the  experiments  so  far  presented.  But 
that  the  first  statement  of  the  thesis  was  not  so  dismally  at  fault 
is  at  once  evidenced  by  the  introspection  on  these  large  transmo- 
grifications of  English  poetry ;  the  tonal  elements  of  the  poetic  line 
do  seem  indeed  to  have  the  power  of  arousing  a  mood  congruous 
to  that  of  the  original  poem,  even  when  torn  from  their  positions 
and  their  rhetorical  anchorage,  and  recast  into  such  form  as  is 
shown  in  the  above  experiments.  The  subjects  did  not  know  at 
the  time  what  poems  were  being  given  them  in  this  potpourric 
manner ;  they  only  knew  it  was  some  poem,  and  that  they  were  to 
introspect  upon  it ;  but  it  was  not  a  guessing  contest  in  any  sense 
of  the  term, — all  intimations  that  it  was  to  be  such  were  stifled 
at  once;  and  to  the  subjects  must  be  given  due  credit  for  their 
admirable  interest  in  the  experiment  from  start  to  finish,  for  in 
such  fragile  matters  as  the  moods  of  the  esthetic  consciousness, 
any  hostility  or  any  lack  of  true  scientific  interest  would  have  been 
fatal  to  the  purpose  in  hand. 

4.    THE  PSYCHO-PHYSIOLOGICAL  VALUE  OF  THE 
POETIC  SUM 

The  question  of  a  tonal  calculus  seems  to  be  the  logical  devel- 
opment of  the  foregoing  experimentation.  It  has  been  shown 
that  short  vowels  and  explosive  consonants  are  provocative  of 
more  motor  arousal  than  the  long  vowels  and  the  liquids.  Strictly 
speaking,  as  has  been  indicated  before,  the  term  "long  vowel"  is 
equivocal.  Except,  of  course  in  vocal  music,  where  the  long  notes 
rightly  function  their  enunciation.  But  if  the  question  be  asked : 
can  we  say  that  this  or  that  number  of  sounds  will  produce  this 
or  that  effect  ?  the  answer  cannot  be  given  in  the  affirmative  with- 
out the  following  reservations :  effects  can  be  calculated,  provided 
the  number  and  arrangement  of  the  sounds  be  taken  into  con- 
sideration. In  the  above  experiments  it  .appeared  that  if  a 
number  of  explosive  sounds  began  the  line  or  the  passage,  then 
the  motor  manifestations  were  intense,  and  also  that  such  mani- 
festations did  not  wear  away  as  soon  as  the  type  of  sound  had 
changed  to  some  less  intense  one.  Changes  in  the  apperceptive 


i28  ROBERT  CHENAULT  GIVLER 

consciousness  did  not  either  run  parallel  with  the  motor  pattern 
nor  did  they  very  often  seem  to  be  influenced  by  the  finger  as 
much  as  by  the  page  of  print.  Yet  the  introspective  and  motor 
results  were  parallel  in  other  ways,  as  has  been  mentioned  so 
frequently  before. 

Only  so  far  as  we  had  data  from  simple  vowel  and  consonant 
experiments,  could  a  tonal  calculus  be  made.  And  so,  when 
even  in  the  simplest  of  the  I-XII  experiments,  there  were  found 
sounds  upon  which  no  previous  experimentation  had  been  done, 
their  values  were  not  known  in  the  same  way  as  the  values  of 
the  simple  sounds  previously  used,  and  no  two  lines  of  the  I-XII 
experiments  could  be  found  which  had  the  same  common  parts 
known  and  unknown,  in  respect  to  psychomotor  value. 

Much  thought  and  time  was  given  to  this  matter,  and  'for  a 
while  it  looked  as  if  we  had  arrived  at  a  solution  of  the  problem 
involved.  But  it  had  to  be  given  up,  and  for  this  reason:  that 
while  in  nearly  all  the  cases  tried,  the  psychomotor  values  of  the 
separate  letter  sounds  as  found  in  the  ninety-six  preliminary 
experiments  upon  the  single  vowels  and  consonants  showed  in 
summation  to  be  equivalent  to  the  psychomotor  values  of  the  first 
three  experiments  performed  upon  each  of  the  poets,  and  that  as 
more  and  more  meaning  came  into  the  experiments  the  sum  was 
affected  by  some  other  element, — yet  inasmuch  as  we  did  not  have 
enough  tonal  elements  to  make  a  full  correlation,  and  inasmuch 
also  as  the  later  poets  experimented  upon  did  not  give  favorable 
results,  presentation  of  data  and  pressing  of  proof  is  withheld 
at  this  time.  We  had  but  four  long  vowels  and  no  short  ones, 
and  it  is  likely  also  that  the  average  motor  effect  of  the  conso- 
nants we  obtained  would  have  been  greatly  modified  by  further 
experimentation  with  other  vowels,  both  long  and  short.  Hence 
this  problem  of  poetic  sums  remains  for  the  time  being  unsolved ; 
were  this  experimentation  to  be  repeated,  that  problem  would 
stand  uppermost  in  the  attempts  at  correlation. 

Only  in  the  longer  passages  does  there  seem  to  be  a  trend 
toward  a  tonal  calculus.  And  here,  the  surprising  thing  is  that 
a  very  small  number  of  explosive  sounds  in  one  passage  over 


ELEMENTS  OF  SPEECH  IN  RELATION  TO  POETRY        129 

those  in  another  produced  a  motor  difference  greater  than  that 
deducible  from  the  single  effects  of  the  elements  involved.  A 
very  rich  experimental  field  lies  right  here,  and,  with  the  method 
and  results  of  this  experiment  herewith  presented,  ripe  and  free 
for  exploitation. 

Futhermore,  with  the  tabulations  of  sound  frequency  before 
him,  one  could  build  up  by  the  method  of  experiments  I-III  such 
experiments  which  would  signify  and  also  contain  the  tonal  body 
of  any  of  the  poets  contained  in  it,  and  then,  by  comparing  the 
results  thus  obtained  with  those  from  large  "ex  poematis"  pas- 
sages see  whether  parallel  effects  were  thus  obtained.  The  tonal 
pattern  of  poetry  is  quite  more  definite  than  hitherto  suspected, 
and  a  poet  may  be  known  by  his  overtones  as  well  as  by  his  sub- 
ject-matter and  stanza  form. 

Little  need  be  said  in  conclusion  other  than  what  we  have 
given  as  results  in  preceding  pages.  Upholders  of  the  tonal 
theory  of  poetry  may  take  a  fastidious  pride  in  some  of  the  find- 
ings of  this  experimentation,  and  recollect  that  Edmund  Burke's 
theory  of  poetry  may  again  be  referred  to  without  apology. 
Certainly  it  is  not  the  intention  of  this  paper  to  neglect  the  formal 
element  of  the  matter,  even  if  the  constancy  of  a  rhythm  form 
was  used  for  the  purpose  of  neglecting  the  form  in  the  final 
account.  It  is  not  an  impossible  assumption  that  poetry  as  well 
as  other  forms  of  art  may  possess  in  each  of  their  leading  fea- 
tures, form  and  content,  a  sufficiency  of  emotional  wealth  to  be 
considered  each  alone  as  able  to  arouse  the  esthetic  consciousness 
to  the  full.  The  union  of  the  two  may  add  nothing  but  unity, — 
and  hence  all  such  experimentation  as  the  above  is  perhaps  more 
of  a  training  in  the  direction  of  attention  than  it  is  a  splitting 
of  the  elements  of  art  asunder.  Nevertheless,  this  must  be  left 
for  the  consideration  of  those  who  are  better  qualified  to  decide 
it  than  the  writer. 

Our  study  is  completed  for  the  present.  So  far  as  we  know, 
no  such  work  has  ever  been  attempted  previously ;  let  us  hope  that 
future  experimentation  along  the  same  line  will  profit  by  our  mis- 


130  ROBERT  CHENAULT  GIVLER 

takes  and  lead  our  results  to  something  finer  and  more  conclusive. 
Eight  years  of  work  culminate  in  the  results  we  have  brought 
forward,  in  which  years  eighteen  thousand  lines  of  poetry  were 
phonetically  measured  and  tabulated,  involving  the  enumeration 
of  nearly  540,000  sounds;  the  measurement  of  the  records  ob- 
tained in  the  laboratory  involved  nearly  300,000  bits  of  data;  the 
computation  of  the  mean,  the  mean  variation  and  the  range  for  all 
the  experiments  and  the  making  of  rank  lists  brings  the  total 
number  of  computations  to  more  than  a  million,  and  with  all  this 
labor,  it  might  seem  to  some  that  far  more  should  have  been 
found  out  concerning  the  psychophysics  of  poetry  than  we  have 
to  offer  in  closing.  But  the  introspective  consciousness  and  the 
motor,  too,  are  not  such  things  as  can  be  coerced  and  cajoled,' — 
all  their  laws  are  by  no  means  sun  clear,  and  to  have  found  out 
something  definite  is  better  than  to  have  been  given  only  shadowy 
hints,  promises,  and  false  signs  to  advance. 

There  were  in  all  fifteen  persons  who  took  part  in  this  investi- 
gation. One  of  these  was  an  instructor  in  the  Department  of 
Psychology.  The  rest  were  mostly  graduate  students  in  the  Lab- 
oratory. Five  were  women  from  Radcliffe  College.  All  were 
trained  introspectors. 

The  following  scheme  shows  what  subjects  took  part  in  the 
investigation  and  for  how  long  time : 

Yr.      I   ABC          F  L  N  T    W    Z    Y 

Yr.    II    A    B     C    D    F    K    L    M  P     S 

Yr.  Ill   B  F    K    L    M 

Thus  three  continued  through  the  whole  period,  and  the  some  five 
subjects  assisted  during  the  last  two  years,  in  which  by  far  the 
most  important  work  was  done. 

The  following  account  briefly  indicates  the  chief  characteristics 
of  the  subjects: 

A.  Predominantly  visual ;  disliked  the  tragic  and  melancholy ; 
closed  his  eyes  whenever  possible;  nodded  head  synchronously 
with  the  tapping;  often  read  in  a  slightly  mournful  tone;  it  was 
usually  unpleasant  for  his  own  personality  to  be  injected  into 
the  imagery ;  good  sense  of  rhythm ;  very  constant  and  steady. 


ELEMENTS  OF  SPEECH  IN  RELATION  TO  POETRY        131 

B.  Visual-motor  type;  enjoyed  the  tragic  and  melancholy  as 
much  as  the  light  romantic;  acquainted  with  English  poetry, 
favored  Byron,  Keats  and  Arnold;  he  alone  of  all  the  subjects 
gave  much  introspection  of  the  sensations  of  the  speech  appara- 
tus; gave  much  introspection;  good  sense  of   rhythm;  steady 
and  constant. 

C.  Motor  type ;  rarely  got  satisfying  imagery  of  any  sort ;  had 
great  difficulty  to  count  the  five  iambics  in  the  "la-mo"  type  of 
experiment ;  sense  of  rhythm  varied  much  with  the  type  of  experi- 
ment; introspection  meagre;  steady  and  constant. 

D.  Very  visual,  with  highly  colored  images;  artistically  gifted 
and  fond  of  poetry ;  enjoyed  the  bizarre  as  well  as  the  sombre ; 
rather  volatile,  but  rebounded  instantly  from  depressed  states; 
strong  sense  of  rhythm. 

F.  Visual-motor;  fond  of  poetry;  good  declaimer,  and  often 
varied  from  a  steady  recitation  of  the  material  experimented 
upon;  articulation  sensations  often  seemed  to  determine  the 
imagery;  German:  had  some  slight  difficulty  in  pronouncing  the 
"th"  and  other  sounds;  strong  sense  of  rhythm;  constant. 

K.  Visual-auditory-motor;  musical  performer;  esthetic;  liked 
the  melancholy;  voice  usually  of  medium  pitch  but  very  low 
intensity ;  pitch  constantly  noticed ;  feeling  of  hoarseness  accom- 
panied low  pitches ;  tapped  very  short  strokes,  often  no  more  than 
12  mm.  in  length;  syncoped  the  tappings  very  frequently;  good 
sense  of  rhythm;  constant. 

L.  Visual-motor;  artistic,  and  fond  of  certain  kinds  of  poetry, 
e.g.  the  sound  of  Shelley's  and  the  content  of  Arnold's ;  introspec- 
tion varied  much,  from  bare  feeling-tone  to  full  auditory-visual- 
motor  content;  very  apt  in  describing  vague  content  by  fitting 
analogy ;  good  sense  of  rhythm ;  steady  and  constant. 

M.  Motor  type ;  practical,  and  impatient  of  most  poetry ;  often 
given  to  intentional  changes  of  extent  of  finger  movement ;  wanted 
objective  finger  control  (the  most  inconsistent  subject  as  far  as 
any  feeling-tone  —motor-discharge  correlation  was  concerned)  ; 
said:  "I  have  a  good  sense  of  rhythm,"  which  did  not  always 
appear. 


132  ROBERT  CHENAULT  GIVLER 

N.  Motor-visual ;  philistinian  toward  most  poetry ;  frequently 
interrupted  the  experiment  with  a  Phillipic  on  the  impracticability 
of  art;  good  introspector ;  good  sense  of  rhythm;  fairly  steady 
and  constant. 

P.  Motor  type;  musical;  singer;  enjoyed  the  less  romantic 
forms  of  poetry;  meagre  imagery;  left-handed  (the  apparatus 
was  accommodated  to  him)  ;  tapped  the  longest  strokes  of  any 
one  (140  mm.)  with  the  smallest  M. V. ;  good  sense  of  rhythm; 
steady  and  constant. 

S.  Visual-motor;  enjoyed  poetry  of  all  kinds;  introspection 
often  by  tactual  analogies ;  had  difficulty  with  the  language,  being 
a  native  of  India;  good  sense  of  rhythm;  fairly  steady  and 
constant. 

T.  Predominantly  visual;  fond  of  all  kinds  of  artistic  work; 
introspection  clear,  often  chromatic;  sense  of  rhythm  well 
marked ;  steady  and  constant. 

W.  Visual;  practical  and  little  acquainted  with  or  apprecia- 
tive of  poetry ;  counting  the  five  iambics  often  very  difficult ;  tap- 
ping never  become  pleasantly  automatic ;  steady  and  constant. 

Z.  Visual-motor;  enjoyed  poetry  and  was  somewhat  gifted  in 
verse-making;  lack  of  imagery  in  the  introspection  often  dis- 
appointing enough  to  change  the  whole  feeling-tone;  steady  and 
constant. 

Y.  Visual;  acquainted  with  English  poetry;  introspection 
rather  meagre;  strong  likes  and  dislikes;  good  sense  of  rhythm; 
steady  and  constant. 

My  thanks  are  due  to  both  Professor  Hugo  Miinsterberg,  di- 
rector of  the  Harvard  Psychological  Laboratory,  and  to  Dr. 
Herbert  Sydney  Langfeld,  Instructor  in  Psychology,  for  their 
kind  and  continual  interest  in  and  criticism  of  this  work.  And 
to  the  subjects  who  took  part  in  the  investigation  much  praise  is 
rightly  bestowed  for  their  perserverance  and  interest. 


Ttl.  XK  PSYCHOLOGICAL  REVIEW  PUBLICATIONS      ,  n 

No.  3  Whole  No.  83 


THE 

Psychological  Monographs 

EDITED    BY 

JAMES  ROWLAND  ANGELL,  UNIVERSITY  OF  CHICAGO 
HOWARD  C.  WARREN,  PRINCETON  UNIVERSITY  (Index) 

JOHN  B.  WATSON,  JOHNS  HOPKINS  UNIVERSITY   (Review)   and 
SHEPHERD  I.  FRANZ,  GOVT.  HOSP.  FOR  INSANE  (Bulletin) 


Standardization  of  Tests  for 
Defective  Children 


By 
CLARA  SCHMITT,  Ph.D. 


PSYCHOLOGICAL  REVIEW   COMPANY 

PRINCETON,  N.  J. 
AND  LANCASTER,  PA. 

AGENTS:  G.  E.  STECHERT  &  CO.,  LONDON  (2  Star  Yard,  Carey  St.,  W.  C.); 
LEIPZIG  (Koenigstr.,  37);  PARIS  (16  rue  de  Conde) 


.111- 


ACKNOWLEDGMENTS 

For  whatever  is  of  value  in  this  work  the  author  is  indebted  to 
many  persons.  It  is  not  possible  to  mention  them  in  a  para- 
graph. In  a  very  special  sense  the  author  wishes  to  express 
gratitude  to  the  following : 

The  Juvenile  Psychopathic  Institute  and  the  director  of  its 
clinic,  Dr.  William  Healy. 

Mrs.  Mary  Chapin  White,  formerly  psychologist  of  the 
Juvenile  Psychopathic  Clinic,  for  help  in  giving  some  of  the  tests. 

To  the  principal  and  teachers  of  the  private  school  where  the 
Healy-Fernald  tests  were  given.  These  teachers  gave  freely  of 
their  time  and  enthusiastic  encouragement.  It  is  regretted  that 
the  policy  of  the  school  regarding  publicity  prevents  a  more  defin- 
ite acknowledgment  of  this  service. 

To  the  principals  and  teachers  of  the  public  schools  where  data 
was  obtained. 

To  Dr.  Charles  H.  Judd  and  Dr.  Frank  N.  Freeman  of  the 
University  of  Chicago  for  helpful  and  patient  guidance  as  teach- 
ers before  this  work  was  undertaken,  and  for  suggestion  and 
criticism  during  its  construction. 

To  the  children  who  so  cheerfully  and  trustfully  did  their  best. 


TABLE  OF  CONTENTS 

PAGE 

I.  Introduction    i 

II.  Historical  development  of  tests  of  mental  measur- 

ment  in  correlation  with  general  intelligence 3 

III.  Binet-Simon  tests  16 

IV.  Fallacies  and  inadequacies  of  the  Binet-Simon  series  52 
V.  Discussion  of  Binet-Simon  tables 68 

VI.  Standardization    and    discussion    of    Healy-Fernald 

tests     81 

VII.  Correlation  of  three  tests  with  school  grade 124 

VIII.  Individual  reactions  to  Healy-Fernald  tests 129 

IX.  Summary  of  Standardization  of  Healy-Fernald  tests  133 

X.  School  subjects  as  material  for  tests  of  mental  ability  137 

XI.  Factors  involved  in  the  mental  classification  of  clinic 

cases    164 

XII.  Qualitative  classification  of  tests. 177 

XIII.  Bibliography    180 


INTRODUCTION 

The  following  study  was  undertaken  to  provide  data  for  the 
standardization  of  the  Healy-Fernald  tests,  described  by  the 
authors  in  Psychological  Monographs,  Vol.  XIII,  No.  2.  It  is 
offered  because  a  standarization  of  these  tests  has  been  asked  for 
by  various  persons  interested  in  the  development  of  clinical 
psychology.  It  is  hoped  that  the  study  will  also  suggest  some 
further  clinical  uses  of  the  Binet-Simon  series.  The  purpose  of 
standardization  is  to  show  the  reactions  of  a  socially  homogeneous 
group  of  individuals  considered  socially  and  pedogogically 
normal,  classified  according  to  certain  principles  discussed  below. 

The  psychological  considerations  which  underlie  the  author's 
classified  evaluation  of  these  tests  has  been  found  of  value  in 
clinics  where  the  mentally  defective  must  be  distinguished  from 
the  mentally  normal,  and  classified  for  the  purposes  of  instruc- 
tion in  the  public  school  and  for  placing  in  public  institutions. 
It  is  hoped  that  the  formulation  of  the  principles  which  have  led  to 
such  classifications  may  prove  to  be  suggestive  to  those  seeking 
further  light  on  the  process  of  mental  diagnosis. 

The  study  is  offered,  however,  with  a  due  appreciation  of  its 
incompleteness.  It  is  desirable  that  a  hundred  or  more  children 
of  each  age  and  grade  of  the  school  should  be  given  the  tests  dis- 
cussed in  the  following  pages,  rather  than  the  twenty  which  it 
was  possible  to  get.  But,  though  the  numbers  are  small,  they  are 
sufficient  to  show  clearly  marked  tendencies  to  specific  types  of 
reaction  for  the  various  grades  and  ages  tested. 

This  work  was  done  by  the  author  while  psychologist  to  the 
Juvenile  Psychopathic  Institute.  This  organization  maintains  a 
clinic  at  the  Juvenile  Court  of  Chicago.  Wherever  in  this  work 
"the  clinic"  is  mentioned  it  is  this  clinic  to  which  reference  is 
made. 

Much  that  is  said  in  this  study  concerning  mental  tests  in  gen- 


2  CLARA  SCHMITT 

eral  and  concerning  many  of  the  individual  tests  is  the  result  of  a 
body  of  experience  gained  in  the  clinic  just  mentioned  and  a 
further  year's  experience  in  the  clinic  of  the  Department  of  Child 
Study  of  the  Board  of  Education  of  Chicago.  Much  of  this 
experience  is  not  amenable  to  statistical  classification.  The  par- 
ticular bit  of  work  which  is  here  used  for  standardization  of  the 
Healy-Fernald  tests  is  especially  suitable  for  this  purpose  because 
it  is  gained  from  a  group  of  socially  homogeneous  subjects. 

The  work  of  the  Juvenile  Court  clinic  is  entirely  with  the  juven- 
ile delinquent;  and  except  for  this  one  characteristic  that  group 
of  cases  is  not  homogeneous.  The  work  in  the  Department  of 
Child  Study  is  with  children  who  for  some  reason  are  reacting 
unsatisfactorily  to  the  school  situation.  This  group  of  subjects 
is  far  from  homogeneous  mentally,  physically  and  socially. 

Since  one  of  the  reasons  for  atypical  social  response  may  be 
mental  defectiveness,  the  reaction  of  what  has  come  to  be  ac- 
cepted by  the  users  of  these  tests  as  mentally  defective  response 
has  been  compared  with  the  mentally  normal  for  each  test.  The 
factors  which  enter  into  consideration  in  classifying  as  mentally 
defective  are  discussed  on  page  164. 

Subjects. — The  children  who  served  as  subjects  for  the  tests 
comprised  the  kindergarten  and  first  six  grades  of  a  private 
school  in  Chicago.  These  children  composed  as  perfectly  homo- 
geneous a  group  as  it  is  probably  possible  to  find  in  a  school. 
They  were  the  children  of  people  of  the  professional  class  mainly. 
A  few  were  children  of  successful  business  men  who  sought  the 
best  obtainable  type  of  education  for  their  children.  The  school 
was  founded  for  the  purpose  of  putting  into  application  the  broad- 
est and  best  conceptions  of  educational  theory  and  practice. 

So  far  as  heredity  in  its  relation  to  social  class  is  concerned 
these  children  were  equally  endowed.  Home  environment  with 
reference  to  educational  endowment  and  stimulus  was  uniform 
as  nearly  as  such  a  matter  may  be  measured.  One  may  assume 
that  the  children  who  belonged  to  the  same  grade  had  had  the 
some  educational  regime  in  home  and  school. 


II 

HISTORICAL  DEVELOPMENT  OF  TESTS  OF  MENTAL 

MEASUREMENT  IN  CORRELATION  WITH 

GENERAL  INTELLIGENCE 

Psychologists  have  for  some  twenty  years  been  concerned 
with  finding  a  measure  of  general  intelligence.  Work  with  dif- 
ferent types  of  tests  under  more  or  less  rigorously  controlled 
laboratory  conditions  has  been  carried  on  with  children  of  dif- 
ferent classes  and  with  college  students,  graded  in  one  way  or  an- 
other according  to  degrees  of  general  intelligence.  Within  the 
last  ten  years  there  has  arisen  the  need  of  application  of  the  work 
of  the  laboratory  psychologist  to  the  practical  work  of  clinics 
for  investigation  of  socially  atypical  individuals.  In  the  United 
States  and  other  countries  the  criminal,  the  mentally  defective,  the 
backward  school  child  and  the  supernormal  child  are  being  inves- 
tigated with  greater  thoroughness  than  ever  before.  To  meet 
this  need  there  have  been  devised  and  invented  tests  for  measur- 
ing mental  ability  of  a  type  quite  different  from  the  tests  devised 
by  the  early  laboratory  psychologists  who  worked  at  the  prob- 
lem. A  short  historical  survey  will  serve  to  characterize  the 
two  groups  of  practical  tests  discussed  in  this  article. 

Only  a  part  of  the  great  mass  of  literature  on  tests  of  general 
intelligence  is  outlined  in  the  following  summary,  namely,  that 
part  which  endeavors  to  establish  a  measure  for  the  general 
intelligence  of  children. 

In  1897  the  American  Psychological  Association  (i)  received 
the  report  of  a  committee  appointed  at  its  previous  meeting  to 
investigate  the  subject  of  physical  and  mental  tests.  This  com- 
mittee agreed  upon  and  reported  a  series  of  tests  which  it  recom- 
mended be  tried  on  college  students  in  the  various  psychological 
laboratories  of  the  country.  The  physical  and  mental  tests  recom- 
mended were  classified  as  follows: 


4  CLARA  SCHM1TT 

Preliminary  Data. — Date  of  birth;  birthplace;  birthplace  of 
father,  birthplace  of  mother;  occupation,  including  class  in  col- 
lege ;  occupation  of  father ;  any  measure  previously  made.  Color 
of  eyes;  color  of  hair;  right  or  left  handed.  Mother's  maiden 
name;  number  of  brothers;  sisters;  order  of  birth;  age  of  parents 
at  birth;  birthplace  and  occupation  of  grandparents.  Assymetry 
of  body;  color  of  eyes,  hair,  complexion;  degeneracy  or  other 
stigma  of  head,  eyes-,  ears,  mouth,  teeth,  hands,  feet,  posture;  gait; 
manners;  coordination  and  speech;  indications  of  intellectual, 
emotional  and  moral  characteristics. 

Physical  Measurements. — Height,  weight,  and  size  of  head. 
Breathing  capacity.  Height  sitting. 

Keenness  of  vision 

Color  vision 

Keenness  of  hearing 

Perception  of  pitch 

Fineness  of  touch 

Sensitiveness  to  pain 

Perception  of  weight  or  force  of  movement 

Dynamometer  pressure  of  right  and  left  hand 

Rate  of  movement 

Fatigue 

Will  power 

Voluntary  attention 

Right  and  left  hand  movement 

Rapidity  of  movement. — Taps  on  telegraph,  short  marks,  tril- 
ling with  two  fingers  or  five. 

Accuracy  of  aim 

Reaction  time  for  sound 

Reaction  time  with  choice. — Card  sorting 

Rate  and  discrimination  of  movement. — Marking  out  100  a's 
in  500  letters,  one  of  a  number  of  geometrical  figures,  or  colors, 
or  pictures,  or  objects. 

Quickness  of  distinction  and  movement. — Rate  at  which  cards 
are  sorted,  combine  with  reaction,  with  choice,  with  effects  of 
practice. 

Perception  of  size. — Draw  a  line  equal  to  a  model  5  cm.  in 
length,  bisect  it,  erect  a  perpendicular  of  the  same  length,  and 
bisect  the  right  hand  angle. 

Perception  of  time. — The  accuracy  with  which  a  standard  time 
can  be  reproduced. 

Memory. — The  accuracy  with  which  eight  numerals  heard  once 
can  be  reproduced,  and  the  accuracy  with  which  a  line  drawn  by 


STANDARDIZATION  OF  TESTS  5 

the  observer  at  the  beginning  of  the  hour  can  be  reproduced  at 
the  end  of  the  hour ;  line  to  be  identified,  not  drawn ;  ten  numer- 
als ;  nine  numerals.  A  combined  test  of  memory,  association  and 
finding  time  as  described  in  the  catalogue  of  the  Columbian  Ex- 
position, accuracy  of  observation  and  recollection  as  proposed 
by  Cattell  and  Bolton. 

Memory  type. — Variation  in  the  use  of  ten  numerals,  compare 
results  for  indication  of  memory  type  and  kind  of  imagery 
preferred. 

Apperception  test  of  Ebbinghaus 

Imagery 

Much,  and  more  than  was  here  recommended,  of  this  work  was 
done  in  the  next  few  years,  and  the  attempt  was  made  to  corre- 
late the  results  with  other  evidences  of  general  intelligence. 

Before  this  time,  however,  some  very  important  pieces  of  work 
had  been  done.  One  of  these  was  the  work  of  Gilbert  (2)  in 
1894.  Gilbert  correlated  weight,  height,  lung  capacity,  simple 
reaction  time,  reaction  time  with  discrimination  and  choice,  and 
time  memory  with  mental  ability.  In  determining  general  mental 
ability  the  teacher's  judgment  was  relied  upon.  Each  teacher 
classified  her  pupils  as  bright,  average  and  dull.  Approximately 
100  children  of  each  age  group  from  six  to  seventeen  were 
measured. 

Porter  (3),  in  1893,  correlated  the  height  and  weight  of  33,500 
school  children  with  age  and  grade. 

Later  Smedley  (4),  in  1900,  correlated  height,  standing  and 
sitting;  weight;  ergograph  and  dynamometer  records,  and  lung 
capacity  with  age  and  school  standing  of  children  between  the 
ages  of  eight  and  eighteen  inclusive. 

West  (5)  correlated  physical  development  and  intellectual  abil- 
ity of  Toronto  school  children.  His  method  of  grouping  for  in- 
tellectual ability  was  according  to  the  teacher's  estimate  of  the 
children  as  good,  average,  and  poor. 

The  investigations  of  the  men  mentioned  and  others  gave  rise 
to  three  different  conclusions  with  regard  to  the  relation  of 
physical  development  and  mental  ability.  Porter,  Smedley  and 
some  others  found  a  positive  correlation  between  physical  develop- 
ment and  mental  ability  of  which  success  in  school  life  was  taken 


6  CLARA  SCHMITT 

as  the  measure.  Gilbert  found  no  correlation  and  West  found  a 
negative  correlation.  These  differences  were  probably  due  to 
different  arrangement  of  data  and  to  different  methods  of  group- 
ing grades  of  intellectual  ability.  Gilbert  did  not  separate  the 
sexes  in  his  tables  and  did  not  state  the  proportions  of  his  dull 
and  bright  groups.  Gilbert  and  West  classified  according  to  the 
teacher's  judgment;  Porter  and  Smedley  according  to  school 
grade  with  reference  to  age.  A  severe  criticism  of  the  method 
of  classification  according  to  teacher's  judgment  may  be  quoted 
from  West's  account  of  his  experience  with  it : 

"It  soon  became  apparent  to  me  that  any  such  classification  of 
children's  mental  ability  would  be  very  greatly  influenced  by  the 
mental  caliber  of  the  teacher  making  the  classification.  .  .  . 
There  were  no  poor  scholars.  The  teachers  were  perfectly  willing 
to  classify  the  scholars  as  of  good  and  average  intelligence  but 
any  intimation  of  poor  or  stupid  scholars  was  taken  as  a  personal 
reflection  upon  the  teacher  of  the  class  in  question.  .  .  .  The 
poor  students  were  no  more  than  a  mere  handful." 

The  method  of  grouping  according  to  success  in  school  life, 
the  method  of  Porter  and  Smedley,  grades  all  children  of  the 
same  age  according  to  the  same  standard.  Though  some  allow- 
ance must  be  made  for  a  small  group  compelled  by  individual  cir- 
cumstances to  residence  in  grades  below  that  of  which  the  mental 
ability  of  each  individual  might  otherwise  permit. 

In  the  reaction  time  tests  Gilbert  thought  to  have  found 
a  correlation  with  intelligence.  He  says,  "The  curves  for  reaction 
time  gave  the  most  positive  results  showing  that  the  brighter  the 
child  the  more  quickly  he  is  able  to  act.  In  discrimination 
the  same  relation  is  true  but  to  a  less  degree.  .  .  .  Of  time 
memory  it  may  be  said  in  general  that  the  brighter  the  child  the 
more  accurate  his  sense  of  time."  An  examination  of  Gilbert's 
tables,  however,  fails  to  support  so  optimistic  a  view  of  the  exist- 
ing correlation.  At  some  ages  the  dull  class  is  superior  to  the 
bright  class,  and  the  differences  between  the  three  classes  meas- 
ured are  everywhere  slight. 

Reaction  time  tests  of  various  kinds  were  tried  out  by  various 
investigators  in  the  following  years.  The  results  of  this  work 


STANDARDIZATION  OF  TESTS  7 

were  reviewed  by  Whipple  (6)  in  1904.  He  distinguished  be- 
tween two  kinds  of  reaction  time  experiment,  the  laboratory  type, 
and  the  anthropometric  type  which  includes  card  sorting  and  sim- 
ilar tests.  The  former  consists  of  work  done  under  rigid  labora- 
tory conditions  upon  subjects  competent  to  make  introspective 
analyses.  The  latter  consists  of  experiments  made  on  children  or 
others  without  introspective  analyses  and  without  practice  in  di- 
reaction  of  attention.  He  pointed  out  that  because  different  ex- 
perimenters obtained  widely  varying  results'  under  the  same 
conditions,  attention  came  to  be  directed  to  the  different  types  of 
individuals,  and  the  reaction  time  experiment  came  to  be  an  ex- 
periment in  reaction  consciousness.  The  anthropometric  type  of 
experiment  he  criticised,  because  the  conditions  under  which  the 
data  were  supplied,  were  so  loosely  controlled  that  one  could 
not  be  sure  what  was  measured.  He  concludes  that  any  reaction 
time  is  conditioned  upon  a  large  number  of  independent  factors 
and  when  these  are  eliminated  or  controlled  in  the  laboratory  "we 
have  left  no  residuum  of  individual  variation  that  can  be  turned 
to  account  in  estimating  the  observer's  general  intelligence  or 
mental  ability." 

In  1901  Wissler  (7)  published  the  results  of  a  long  series  of 
tests  and  anthropometrical  measurements  made  under  the  direc- 
tion of  Cattell  upon  students  of  Columbia  University  for  a  period 
of  seven  years.  The  results  of  these  tests  were  correlated  with 
class  standing.  The  general  conclusions  were  that  the  laboratory 
mental  tests  show  little  correlation  in  the  case  of  college  students ,- 
that  the  physical  tests  show  a  general  tendency  to  correlate  with 
themselves  but  only  to  a  very  slight  degree  with  mental  tests; 
that  the  markings  of  students  in  college  classes  correlate  with 
themselves  to  a  considerable  degree  but  not  with  the  tests  made 
in  the  laboratory. 

Griffing  (8),  1895,  and  others  investigated  the  subject  of  at- 
tention with  reference  to  general  intelligence.  In  general  some 
form  of  the  tachistoscope  was  used.  Griffing's  conclusions  rep- 
resent the  general  concensus  of  opinion  among  these  investigators. 
He  said,  "I  found  that  those  rated  'A'  for  mental  capacity  by  the 
teachers  on  an  A  B  C  basis,  had  somewhat  higher  averages  than 


8  CLARA  SCHMITT 

the  others.  .  .  .  There  are,  however,  marked  exceptions.  .  .  .  Those 
marked  'A'  by  their  teachers  for  attention  in  class  also  excelled 
the  others,  but  here  also  I  found  decided  exceptions.  Many 
pupils  must  have,  therefore,  good  powers  of  attention,  even  when 
they  show  no  evidence  of  them  to  their  teachers." 

In  1904  Spearman  (9)  made  a  critical  analysis  of  the  methods 
of  work  in  the  determination  of  correlations  of  various  tests  with 
general  intelligence.  With  a  more  exact  mathematical  formula 
for  the  calculation  of  correlations  and  by  the  use  of  more  factors 
for  the  determination  of  general  intelligence  he  found  large  corre- 
lations in  tests  of  discrimination  of  grays,  and  weight  and  pitch, 
with  general  intelligence. 

In  1909  Burt  (10)  correlated  the  general  intelligence  of 
two  sets  of  English  schoolboys  with  tests  of  discrimination  of  two 
points  upon  the  skin,  of  lifted  weights,  of  pitch,  and  of  length 
of  lines.  To  these  he  added  two  motor  tests,  tapping  and  card 
dealing;  two  sensori-motor  tests,  card  sorting  and  alphabet  find- 
ing; tests  of  immediate  memory  of  concrete  words,  abstract 
words  and  nonsense  syllables;  the  tracing  of  a  geometrical  pat- 
tern seen  in  a  mirror,  a  test  of  the  power  to  acquire  new  co- 
ordinations; the  reproduction  from  memory  of  a  pattern  of 
spots  presented  by  the  tachistoscope  upon  squared  paper;  and  a 
test  of  voluntary  attention,  which  consisted  of  pricking  an 
irregular  line  of  dots  passing  rapidly  before  the  subject. 

Great  care  was  exercised  by  Burt,  in  accordance  with  the 
recommendations  of  Spearman,  in  the  mathematical  work  of 
correlating  the  test  findings  with  general  intelligence.  The  latter 
was  estimated  by  the  headmasters  of  the  schools  from  which  the 
reagents  came.  The  conclusions  at  which  Burt  arrived  are  as 
follows : 

Of  the  simple  sensory  tests,  tactile,  weight,  pitch,  and  length 
of  line  discrimination,  he  says,  "There  appears  to  be  no  general 
connection  between  intelligence  and  capacity  to  discriminate 
weights;  any  connections  between  intelligence  and  tactile  dis- 
crimination, if  it  exists,  is  of  the  slightest;  there  is  considerable 
connection  between  intelligence  and  capacity  to  discriminate 
undoubted  general  connection  between  intelligence  and  visual 
discrimination  of  lengths.  .  .  .  ' 


STANDARDIZATION  OF  TESTS  9 

Of  the  motor  tests,  tapping  and  card  dealing  he  says :  "Motor 
tests  seem  to  have  a  higher  correlation  with  intelligence  than 
sensory  tests.  But  where  rapidity  is  due  to  frequent  practice.  .  . 
the  correlations  with  intelligence  and  other  tests  are  reduced, 
abolished,  or  inverted.  Thus  so  far  as  motor  rapidity  is  the  func- 
tion of  temporary  'facilitation'  of  the  paths  of  neural  discharge 
it  appears  also  to  be  a  function  of  intelligence,  while  so  far  as  it  is 
a  function  of  permanent  'canalisation'  of  those  paths  it  is  but 
slightly  or  inversely  related  to  intelligence."  This  latter  con- 
clusion was  the  result  of  disturbance  of  correlational  results 
among  a  group  of  practiced  card  players. 

Of  the  two  sensori-motor  tests,  one  for  the  sorting  of  cards 
according  to  color,  the  other  for  selecting  a  complete  alphabet 
from  a  mixture  of  two  alphabets,  Burt  says :  "Depending  as  they 
do  for  their  performance  upon  processes  of  a  more  complex 
nature  and  a  higher  mental  level,  tests  combining  perception 
with  motor  reaction  seem  to  involve  the  intelligence  to  a  still 
higher  degree  than  relatively  simple  sensory  or  motor  tests.  Of 
the  two  above  discussed  the  alphabet  seems  to  be,  in  practice,  far 
the  more  efficient." 

Immediate  memory  was  tested  by  the  use  of  sets  of  words  of 
abstract  significance,  of  concrete  significance,  and  nonsense 
syllables.  The  correlation  with  general  intelligence  found  by 
Burt  between  concrete  memory,  abstract  memory,  and  nonsense 
memory  was  .58,  .48,  .43  respectively  for  one  group  of  boys  and 
.84,  .78,  .75  for  another  group.  "Thus  the  memory  for  abstract 
words  does  not  show  a  higher,  but  a  lower  correlation,  with  intel- 
ligence. .  .  .  The  introduction  of  difficult  vocables,  whether  ab- 
stract nouns  or  meaningless  syllables,  proves  in  both  groups  to 
be  on  the  whole  a  distracting  element." 

In  the  mirror  test,  a  pattern  is  traced  which,  with  the  hand 
doing  the  work,  is  seen  only  in  a  mirror.  This  tests  the  subject's 
ability  to  readjust  certain  already  learned  eye  and  hand  coordina- 
tions to  a  changed  situation.  Burt  found  many  difficulties  in 
the  mechanical  operation  of  this  test  as  well  as  in  the  method  of 
measuring  results.  In  his  judgment  it  was  a  test  which  with 
further  perfection  would  be  of  great  practical  value.  He,  how- 


io  CLARA  SCHMITT 

ever,  sums  up  the  factors  which  would  complicate  and  make 
uncertain  the  results  of  this  test  in  practice  when  he  accounts  for 
the  divergence  between  his  two  groups  of  the  correlational  fig- 
ures, .67  and  .54.  "The  divergence  between  the  two  schools  is 
largely  due  to  the  fact  that  four  of  the  Preparatory  schoolboys 
had  had  previous  practice  at  an  analogous  task  in  the  form  of  a 
not  very  common  parlor  pastime.  Only  one  of  the  thirty  Elemen- 
tary boys  had  done  any  similar  exercise  before.  The  divergence 
might  also  be  in  part  attributed  to  a  greater  familiarity  with 
the  use  of  the  mirror  among  boys  of  a  higher  class  as  compared 
with  boys  of  a  lower  status.  A  similar  factor  apparently  operated 
when  the  test  was  applied  to  children  of  the  opposite  sex,  though 
subsequent  application  to  very  young  children,  and  to  adults, 
have  led  me  to  wonder  whether  we  are  not  dealing  with  one  of 
the  uninvestigated  innate  differences  between  the  sexes." 

The  spot  pattern  test  was  given  in  a  dark  room  by  means  of  a 
tachistoscope.  The  pattern  was  shown  as  many  times  as  it  was 
necessary  for  the  subject  to  learn  to  reproduce  it  correctly.  The 
difficulties  in  the  use  of  the  tachistoscope  in  practical  work  are 
indicated  when  he  says,  "The  tachistoscope  was  found  to  require 
a  larger  amount  of  experience  on  the  part  of  both  subjects  and 
operators,  than  any  of  the  other  tests,  except  perhaps  those  in- 
volving sensory  discrimination.  .  .  .  The  first  series  of  all  had  to 
be  rejected  as  worthless,  owing  partly  to  the  irrelevant  excite- 
ment aroused  in  the  subjects  by  the  'electrical  flash'  as  the  boys 
named  it.  ...  At  this  school  we  were  not  able  to  obtain  the 
complete  darkness  and  silence  procured  at  the  other  in  our  ex- 
temporised dark  room,  and  consequently  the  reliability  coefficient 
and  the  raw  correlations  with  intelligence  are  not  so  high."  The 
coefficient  obtained  was  .76  and  .75  for  the  two  groups. 

Burt's  test  of  sustained  attention  consisted  of  pricking  dots 
irregularly  arranged  upon  a  strip  of  paper  which  passed  before 
the  subject.  The  number  of  dots  per  minute  which  the  student 
marked  constituted  a  measure  of  his  ability.  The  correlation  for 
this  test  with  general  intelligence  was  found  to  be  .75  and  .96 
for  the  two  groups  of  boys. 


STANDARDIZATION  OF  TESTS  11 

Of  the  practical  significance  of  these  tests  Burt  says :  "Of  the 
twelve  tests  six  furnish  coefficients  below  .50  and  six  above  .50. 
The  former  six — the  simple  sensory  and  motor  tests — are  thus 
of  little  use  in  the  empirical  diagnosis  of  intelligence.  Among 
the  latter  six,  no  single  test,  at  any  rate  in  its  present  form,  can 
be  claimed  as  a  self-sufficient  instrument  for  measuring  and 
detecting  ability  in  individuals.  But  they  indicate  the  direction 
in  which  such  a  test  may  hopefully  be  sought.  .  .  .  McDougall's 
dotting  machine  seems  to  be  the  most  scientific.  Where  the 
external  conditions  could  be  kept  most  uniform,  .  .  .  both  the 
amalgamated  and  the  average  raw  coefficients  reached  .84.  Such 
uniformity  is  difficult  in  more  extensive  work,  and  the  ensuing 
variety  in  attention  and  fatigue  affect  the  performances  with  this 
test.  Moreover,  its  figures  are  less  discriminative  than  either  of 
the  other  three.  By  increasing  the  number  of  spots  in  the 
pattern  the  tachistoscope  test  may  be  made  to  differentiate  with 
almost  any  degree  of  minuteness.  .  .  .  It  is  a  slow  test,  however, 
and  without  repetition  scarcely  reliable.  And  it  calls  for  some 
experience  both  on  the  part  of  the  boys  to  grasp  the  nature  of 
the  task,  and  on  the  part  of  the  experimenter  to  manipulate 
the  apparatus  with  regularity.  .  .  .  The  mirror  test  can  be  pro- 
cured with  but  little  trouble  and  expense,  and  needs  no  trained 
superintendent.  It,  too,  requires  further  improvements,  espe- 
cially in  procedure  and  calculations,  to  eliminate  the  influence  of 
possible  previous  practice,  and  to  elicit  more  completely  the  sig- 
nificance of  the  figures  observed.  If  called  upon  to  recommend 
a  simple  test  for  immediate  use  upon  untrained  subjects,  I  should 
be  inclined  to  advocate  the  alphabet  test  as  perhaps  the  simplest 
and  most  satisfactory  test  of  all." 

The  work  of  Burt  has  been  so  fully  recorded  because  it  is  the 
broadest  and  most  careful  attempt  to  correlate  the  results  of  tests 
with  general  intelligence.  In  this  work  many  considerations  were 
taken  into  account  in  constructing  a  scale  of  general  intelligence 
of  the  subjects.  Great  care  was  taken  in  the  management  of 
the  tests  themselves  and!  the  mathematical  correlations  were 
worked  out  with  accuracy.  The  conclusions  drawn  of  the  vari- 


12  CLARA  SCHMITT 

cms  tests  are  valuable  from  the  standpoint  of  an  interpretation 
of  intelligence.  For  various  reasons,  however,  the  results  can 
not  be  immediately  applied  to  clinical  work.  The  reasons  may 
be  grouped  under  five  heads. 

The  first  and  most  important  is  the  measurement  of  the  tests 
against  time.  It  is  the  experience  of  the  writer  and  others 
in  the  work  of  children's  clinics  that  time  within  the  limits  of 
rigid  laboratory  procedure  can  not  be  taken  as  the  measure  of 
the  subject's  ability  with  a  particular  test.  This  is  because  of 
the  peculiar  demands  of  such  a  clinic.  One  wishes  the  child 
to  be  unaffected  by  any  feelings  of  fear  or  anxiety  or  strange- 
ness with  the  situation  when  he  comes  up  for  examination.  As 
far  as  possible  he  should  not  know  that  he  is  being  examined. 
He,  therefore,  should  not  be  subjected  to  the  anxious  desire 
to  make  good  time  in  any  thing  he  is  doing.  There  are  few 
tests  which  can  at  all  lend  themselves  to  such  measurement, 
since  in  any  test  which  can  be  useful  as  a  measure  of  intelligence 
or  which  can  show  the  child's  intelligence  functioning,  there 
are  involved  perceptual  or  other  types  of  discrimination  which 
may  be  interfered  with  if  the  child  anxiously  desires  to  make 
a  good  time  record.  This  interference  with  thought  processes 
may  cause  the  final  result  to  be  a  misleading  and  perhaps  unfair 
judgment  of  his  general  intelligence.  Only  such  discriminations 
as  are  habitual  with  him  and  therefore  make  little  demand  upon 
attention  can  be  measured  against  time.  Under  such  circum- 
stances one  does  not  know  what  is  being  measured. 

The  second  reason  for  the  impracticability  of  Burt's  work  is 
the  fact  that  with  tests  which  correlate  most  highly  with  gen- 
eral intelligence,  the  use  of  apparatus  is  necessary.  Burt,  him- 
self, showed  how  the  tachistoscope  mechanism  interfered  with 
attention  to  the  object  of  the  test  itself.  In  much  larger  meas- 
ure would  such  a  piece  of  apparatus  be  a  stumbling  block  in 
.  a  clinic  where  defective  individuals  are  examined.  As  was 
remarked  above,  one  wishes  the  child  to  be  unaffected  by  any 
feelings  of  fear  or  strangeness  with  the  new  and  usually  strange 
situation  into  which  he  is  thrust  when  he  comes  to  a  psychological 
clinic  for  examination.  A  piece  of  strange  apparatus  will  so 


STANDARDIZATION  OF  TESTS  13 

fill  his  mind  with  fear  or  speculation  as  to  its  object  and  its 
affect  upon  himself,  that  he  may  not  act  normally.  The  fact 
also  that  much  practice  is  needed  on  the  part  of  the  subject 
to  use  a  piece  of  apparatus  must  put  it  out  of  consideration 
for  the  practical  clinician. 

The  third  reason  for  discarding  some  of  these  tests  is  that 
previous  practice  may  vitiate  results.  What  Burt  has  said 
with  regard  to  the  mirror  test  and  card  dealing  test  has  already 
been  quoted.  No  tests  except  those  on  reading,  arithmetic  or 
other  subjects  of  cultural  value  can  be  used  in  a  practical  clinic 
which  in  any  way  may  have  been  the  subject  of  practice  on 
the  part  of  the  child,  and  these  only  if  the  examiner  knows  the 
extent  of  the  child's  experience  with  them. 

The  fourth  point  of  error  in  applying  the  work  of  Burt, 
and  all  other  works  of  its  type,  to  the  examination  of  atypical 
children  is  that  it  does  not  take  into  account  the  child's  motive 
for  action.  In  child  life  there  are  in  general  two  motives  for 
voluntary  activity.  One  of  these  is  the  play  motive,  in  which 
the  child  voluntarily  seeks  the  end  to  be  attained.  The  other  is 
the  social  motive  of  pleasing  associates,  who  may  demand  ends 
which  he  would  not  voluntarily  seek  to  attain.  In  a  psyscho- 
logical  laboratory  the  motives  of  the  adult  subject  and  the 
observer  are  the  same, — the  production  of  scientific  data.  In 
such  case  the  subject  lends  himself  willingly  to  any  conditions 
imposed.  Knowing  the  end  of  the  experiment  he  is  able  to 
direct  attention  to  the  attainment  of  that  end  and  away  from 
the  distracting  elements  of  unusual  conditions.  It  is  otherwise 
with  the  child.  With  him  the  motive  most  conducive  to  natural 
reaction,  uncomplicated  by  disturbing  emotions,  is  the  play 
motive. 

The  fifth  reason  for  the  lack  of  applicability  of  this  type 
of  work  to  a  practical  clinic  is  that  it  has  been  done  with 
subjects  of  so  high  a  type  of  mentality  that  the  results  are  of 
little  value  in  measuring  low  types.  A  mathematical  statement 
of  the  correlations  of  a  test  to  the  general  ability  of  such 
subjects  as  form  the  reagents  for  experiments  under  laboratory 
conditions  can  have  little  significance  in  a  clinic  for  defective 


I4  CLARA  SCHMITT 

or  abnormal  individuals.  The  subjects  for  laboratory  experi- 
ments are  in  general  of  a  high  type  of  intelligence.  Among 
Burt's  subjects  there  was  only  one  defective  child.  In  a  clinic 
no  child  typical  of  the  average  in  the  social  situation  in  which 
he  finds  himself,  ever  comes  up  for  examination.  Necessarily 
in  some  realm  of  social,  mental  or  physical  functioning,  the 
child  to  be  examined  is  abnormal  or  he  would  not  be  brought 
for  examination. 

A  part  of  Burt's  work,  however,  can  be  of  greater  value 
for  clinic  purposes  than  his  correlations  led  him  to  believe. 
Such  simple  sensori-motor  tests  as  card  sorting,  or  as  Whipple 
designated  them,  the  anthropometrical  reaction  tests,  have  been 
found  by  him  and  other  laboratory  experimenters  to  correlate 
little  with  general  intelligence.  The  reason  for  this  is  that 
even  with  the  least  intelligent  subjects  these  tests  fall  well 
within  the  limit  of  their  intelligence,  and,  therefore,  can  not 
form  a  measure  of  the  mental  ability  of  those  particular  sub- 
jects. With  many  subjects  of  the  practical  clinic,  however, 
such  simple  tests  may  be  of  great  value.  If  it  is  found  that 
the  child  can  do  nothing  more  complex  than  sort  cards  according 
to  color  or  geometrical  form,  or  whatever  type  of  discrimination 
is  employed,  this,  in  part  at  least,  establishes  a  measure  of  his 
mental  ability.  If  it  is  found  that  he  can  not  do  even  these 
tests  then  others  of  a  simpler  nature  still  must  be  used  as  a 
measure. 

Viewed  from  the  standpoint  of  such  use,  the  simple  "anthro- 
pometrical reaction"  tests  are  of  great  value  as  measures  of 
certain  elements  of  general  intelligence.  For  use  in  a  practical 
clinic  any  test  may  serve  as  a  measure,  in  whole  or  in  part,  if 
it  really  does  mark  off  a  range  of  intellectual  activity.  However, 
no  test  can  be  of  such  general  use  as  Burt  thought  possible  of 
the  alphabet  sorting  test  which  he  considered  most  valuable  as 
a  test  of  mental  measurement.  The  highly  intelligent  child 
can  accomplish  tests  of  far  greater  complexity;  the  low  grade 
child  may  be  unable  to  make  such  fine  discriminations.  This 
test,  therefore  can  not  serve  as  a  measure  of  these  two  grades 
of  mental  ability. 


STANDARDIZATION  OF  TESTS  15 

To  sum  up,  the  clinic  must  discard  time  for  the  most  part 
as  an  important  factor  in  the  measurement  of  results.  This 
point  is  discussed  further  below  in  connection  with  certain  tests. 
Some  other  measure,  preferably  a  qualitative  one,  must  be  sub- 
stituted for  this  quantitative  one.  The  clinic  must  discard 
rigorous  laboratory  conditions  and  adjust  its  tests  to  conditions 
more  in  conformity  with  those  of  everyday  life.  It  must  dis- 
card such  apparatus  as  requires  practice  on  the  part  of  the 
subject,  or  as  is  not  directly  connected  with  the  object  of  the 
test. 

It  was  such  considerations  as  the  foregoing  which  led  Binet 
and  Simon  in  1904  to  compile  the  series  of  tests  which  have 
since  been  rearranged  and  modified  into  the  series  of  1911  (n). 
In  1904  it  was  required  that  the  mentally  defective  children  in 
the  public  schools  of  Paris  be  segregated  after  individual  ex- 
amination. Binet,  who  had  contributed  in  large  measure  through 
his  laboratory  experiments  to  the  psychology  of  mental  tests 
and  mental  measurement,  undertook  to  arrange  a  series  of 
tests  capable  of  practical  application  to  young  children.  These 
tests  eliminated  to  a  large  extent,  the  quantitative  measurement 
of  results  and  substituted  a  qualitative  measure.  The  require- 
ment of  laboratory  conditions  was  discarded  in  favor  of  a  situa- 
tion more  in  accord  with  the  normal  every  day  life  of  the  child. 


Ill 

THE  BINET-SIMON  TESTS 

This  series  of  tests  marks  the  real  beginning  of  the  applica- 
tion of  psychological  tests  to  the  practical  work  of  discriminating 
defectives  from  normal  human  beings.  Binet  revised  his  first 
scale  in  1908.  In  this  form  it  was  used  largely  by  many  ex- 
perimenters in  Europe  and  America.  Later,  in  1911,  taking 
account  of  the  criticism  arising  from  these  experiences  with 
his  scale,  Binet  again  made  a  revision.  Several  experimenters 
have  added  materially  to  our  knowledge  of  the  usefulness  of 
this  series  of  tests  under  the  conditions  of  practical  work. 
Bobertag  (12)  has  made  a  thorough  analysis  of  the  psychologi- 
cal significance  of  each  test  and  applied  the  series  to  a  group 
of  German  children.  Goddard  (13)  applied  it  to  four  hundred 
feeble-minded  children  of  the  Vineland  School  for  the  feeble- 
minded, and  to  two  thousand  public  school  children.  Kuhlman 
(14)  used  the  tests  in  the  institution  for  the  feeble-minded  at 
Faribault,  Minnesota.  Terman  and  Childs  (15)  applied  the 
tests  to  a  large  group  of  normal  children  in  California  and  sug- 
gested certain  revisions  and  additions  to  them. 

In  the  following  discussion  it  is  hoped  to  show  something 
of  the  psychological  significance  of  the  individual  tests  of  this 
series  and  its  value  in  clinical  work.  Only  so  much  of  the 
description  of  each  test  and  its  application  is  given  as  will 
indicate  the  nature  of  the  test.  In  some  cases  a  more  elaborate 
statement  will  be  given  where  the  author  suggests  a  wider  use 
of  certain  tests  than  that  recommended  by  Binet.  The  dis- 
cussion begins  with  the  tests  for  five  years  because  the  writer's 
clinical  experience  with  children  under  five  years  of  age  has 
been  small. 

CHILDREN  OF  FIVE  YEARS 

I.  Compare  two  weights* — Four  boxes  in  sets  of  two  are 
used.  They  are  the  same  in  appearance  and  volume  and  weigh 


STANDARDIZATION  OF  TESTS  17 

respectively,  three  grams  and  twelve  grams;  six  grams  and 
fifteen  grams.  The  first  two  are  placed  on  the  table  before  the 
child  and  he  is  asked  to  lift  them  both  and  hand  to  the  ex- 
perimenter the  one  which  is  the  heavier.  This  tests  the  child's 
ability  to  compare  two  sensations  and  form  a  judgment  concern- 
ing them.  The  test  is  concerned  with  the  ability  of  the  subject 
to  make  such  comparison  and  not  at  all  with  the  keenness  of 
his  discrimination  of  differences  of  weight.  The  subject  must 
also  exhibit  ability  to  interpret  and  classify  his  sensations  in 
language.  In  the  tenth  year  when  weights  are  arranged  in  serial 
order  the  later  test  deals  more  with  sensory  discrimination. 

Bobertag  points  out  the  interesting  significance  of  correspond- 
ence of  this  psychological  procedure  with  the  highest  intellectual 
accomplishments  in  science  and  practical  life.  A  quantity  of 
sensations  are  presented  to  the  individual  under  normal  life 
conditions,  and  these  are  arranged  in  certain  classifications  of 
different  kinds.  In  this  way  order  is  established  in  the  mental 
life.  To  be  able  to  isolate  in  consciousness  one  type  of  sensa- 
tion from  all  others  and  to  arrange  its  variations  in  a  serial 
order  from  little  to  great  is  the  first  necessity  of  conscious 
intellectual  life. 

Defective  children  will  hand  to '  the  experimenter  the  one 
which  happens  to  be  the  most  convenient  to  pick  up,  or  will 
do  nothing  at  all  because  of  lack  of  comprehension  of  the 
problem. 

II.  Copies  a  square. — To  pass  this  test  the  subject  must  com- 
mand three  abilities.  First,  the  comprehension  of  the  square 
as  such,  a  perceptual  discrimination.  The  same  type  of  dis- 
crimination is  tested  with  the  Seguin  form  board  and  sorting 
cards.  Second,  the  muscular  control  necessary  to  make  lines 
of  equal  length  or  approximately  so;  and  third,  the  ability  to 
cooperate  the  two  foregoing  processes  for  the  production  of 
the  final  result.  If  the  test  is  not  passed  it  may  mean  that  the 
child  has  not  a  comprehension  of  the  distinguishing  character- 
istics of  the  model  before  him,  or  it  may  mean  that  he  has  not 

*  The  descriptions  of  tests  follow  the  translation  by  Town."     The  Binet 
quotations  are  from  the  same  source. 


i8  CLARA  SCHMITT 

the  motor  control  which  will  enable  him  to  draw  a  model  which 
he  recognizes,  or  he  has  not  made  the  requisite  mental  coopera- 
tion. Binet  directs  that  the  child  be  required  to  copy  the  square 
with  ink,  not  pencil.  This  direction  would  indicate  that  the 
test  was  intended  to  be  one  of  motor  control  as  well  as  one  of 
intellectual  comprehension,  since  the  use  of  the  pen  adds  a 
motor  difficulty. 

The  writer  has  seen  defective  children  who  were  able  to 
distinguish  a  square  from  a  circle,  or  some  other  form,  as  was 
shown  in  the  card  sorting  test  in  which  cards  were  sorted  ac- 
cording to  the  geometrical  forms  upon  them;  but  were  unable 
to  initiate  the  process  of  step  three  sufficiently  well  to  draw  a 
recognizable  copy  of  the  model.  Their  copy  was  a  mere  scribble. 

There  also  come  to  the  clinic  children  suffering  from  nervous 
derangements,  who  cannot  control  the  hands  sufficiently  to  draw 
a  straight  or  approximately  straight  line,  or  draw  one  of  the 
length  desired,  and  who  therefore  also  fail  to  make  right  angles ; 
but  who,  it  seems  evident  from  other  tests  which  they  pass, 
and  especially  from  the  dissatisfaction  which  they  show  with 
the  result  of  their  efforts  with  this  one  possess  the  two  other 
abilities  necessary. 

III.  Repeat  a  sentence  of  ten  syllables. — Binet  says  of 
this  test,  "After  the  comprehension  of  words,  the  next  step  in  the 
development  of  language  is  not,  as  one  might  think,  the  verbal 
expression  of  thought  and  the  naming  of  desired  objects,  but 
a  repetition  of  words  heard.  It  is  easier,  approximately,  to 
echo  a  word  than  to  use  it  independently,  to  pass  from  an 
idea  to  a  word."  In  giving  this  test  one  says  to  the  child,  "I 
shall  now  say  something  to  which  you  must  listen  carefully 
and  then  say  it  just  exactly  as  I  do."  Binet  permits  no  error 
whatever  in  the  reproduction  of  the  sentences.  The  series  of 
sentences  given  by  Binet  are : 

I  am  cold  and  hungry. 

My  name  is  Gaston.     Oh!  the  naughty  dog. 

Let  us  go  for  a  long  walk.     Give  me  the  pretty  little  bonnet. 

Bobertag  uses  the  following  list: 
I  am  a  good  child. 


STANDARDIZATION  OF  TESTS  ^ 

I   have  a  pretty  dog. 

I  sit  upon  a  chair. 

My  brother  has  gone  away. 

I  will  go  to  visit  my   father  tomorrow. 

I  have  bought  myself  a  new  suit.. 

We  have  not  yet  done  our  school  work. 

Now  we  will  go  together  to  take  a  walk. 

I  have  said  to  my  brother  that  he  should  visit  me. 

When  we   have   done   our  work   then   we   may   play. 

Bobertag  grades  these  from  6  to  16  syllables.  The  English 
translation  makes  the  number  of  syllables  in  some  cases  slightly 
different.  In  such  sentences  as  Bobertag's  the  writer  finds  a 
certain  advantage  over  those  of  Binet.  With  the  Binet  set  one 
necessarily  drops  the  voice  and  pauses  at  the  end  of  the  short 
sentences,  which  constitute  the  set  of  ten  syllables  or  more, 
and  the  child,  on  the  qui  vive  to  reproduce  immediately,  begins 
to  do  so  as  soon  as  he  hears  the  drop  of  the  voice,  or  the  pause, 
not  realizing  that  the  set  is  not  yet  finished.  There  is  probably 
a  difference  in  the  memory  process  of  the  two  types  of  sentences. 
Binet  assumes  that  the  child  does  not  attend  to  the  idea  but  only 
to  the  words  and  reproduces  them.  The  Binet  sets  contain  more 
than  one  idea.  That  the  process  of  remembering  the  sentence 
which  is  used  for  the  expression  of  one  idea,  such  as  those  of 
Bobertag,  is  easier  than  that  of  remembering  a  sentence  of  the 
same  number  of  syllables  which  expresses  two  or  more  ideas 
is  indicated  by  the  fact  that  Binet  found  the  five-year  old 
child  generally  able  to  remember  no  more  than  a  sentence  of  ten 
syllables,  but  Bobertag  found  that  sentences  of  sixteen  syllables 
were  not  too  difficult. 

IV.  Counts  four  pennies. — This  test  shows  whether  or 
not  the  child  has  learned  this  series  of  four  terms  and  has 
related  the  terms  of  the  series  to  four  like  objects.  Children 
may  learn  the  counting  series  without  relating  it  to  anything; 
that  is,  without  ever  counting  anything.  General  observation 
shows  this  to  be  the  case  with  young  children.  A  child  of 
four,  who  could  count  to  five  as  a  mere  word  series,  was  observed 


20  CLARA  SCHMITT 

by  the  writer  to  make  for  himself  the  discovery  that  he  could 
relate  this  series  to  five  objects;  upon  the  first  occasion  his  five 
toes.  To  count  something,  then,  is  a  step  in  advance  of  merely 
counting.  Defective  young  children  in  the  schools  relate  the 
counting  series  imperfectly,  or  not  at  all,  to  the  objects  before 
them. 

Some  defective  children  will  relate  the  counting  series  per- 
fectly to  a  series  of  objects;  that  is,  they  count  correctly  a 
row  of  objects  but  have  no  appreciation  of  the  number  concept 
involved.  If,  for  instance,  after  the  child  has  counted  a  series 
of  four  objects,  he  is  asked,  "How  many  are  there?"  the  answer 
may  be,  "ten."  If  a  row  of  objects,  say  two  or  three  is  placed 
before  him,  and  he  is  asked  to  tell  how  many  are  there  he  wi1! 
again  count  correctly  as  a  series  and  relate  the  series  correctly 
to  the  objects  before  him  and  answer  wildly,  "seven"  or  "nine." 

The  normal  child  of  this  age  is  able  not  only  to  count  cor- 
rectly, but  also  to  understand  that  his  counting  numbers.  He 
does  not  make  so  erratic  an  answer  as  has  been  indicated  of 
the  defective  child.  Binet  and  his  followers  have  made  this 
test  rather  a  vague  one  by  insisting  that  the  child  be  asked  to 
count  four  pennies  and  by  pointing  out  that  it  is  necessary  for 
him  to  count  some  such  series  of  objects  which  are  of  interest 
to  him.  The  writer  finds  that  if  the  child  can  count  at  all 
he  can  and  is  willing  to  count  anything.  The  writer  generally 
uses  a  row  of  small  circles  upon  a  sheet  of  paper.  The  child 
always  counts  these  as  willingly  as  he  would  count  pennies,  or 
other  objects. 

V.  Game  of  patience  with  two  pieces. — For  this  test 
an  oblong  card  is  cut  along  the  diagonal,  making  two  triangular 
pieces.  An  uncut  card  is  placed  on  the  table  at  the  same  time 
as  the  pieces  of  the  cut  card  and  the  child  is  told  that  a  card 
like  the  one  before  him  was  cut  in  two  and  that  he  may  arrange 
the  pieces  as  before  it  was  cut.  This  tests  his  ability  to  con- 
struct from  a  given  bit  of  material  a  product  to  correspond 
to  a  given  model.  Failure  to  do  this  is  significant  of  the  child's 
lack  of  constructive  ability  to  the  extent  that  the  complexity 
of  this  work  permits  it  to  be  measured. 


STANDARDIZATION  OF  TESTS  21 

Binet  says  of  this  test,  "After  the  operation  is  analyzed,  it 
is  found  to  consist  of  the  following  elements :  i .  To  keep  in 
mind  the  end  to  be  attained,  that  is  to  say,  the  figure  to  be 
formed;  it  is  necessary  to  comprehend  this  end,  it  is  necessary 
also  to  think  about  it,  not  to  lose  sight  of  it.  2.  To  try  dif- 
ferent combinations,  under  the  influence  of  this  directing  idea, 
which  often  guides  the  efforts  of  the  child  though  he  be  un- 
conscious of  the  fact.  3.  To  judge  the  formed  combination, 
compare  it  with  the  model,  and  decide  whether  it  is  the  correct 
one." 

In  the  writer's  opinion  the  cut  pieces  should  be  laid  with  the 
two  long  sides  of  the  original  rectangle  parallel  with  each  other, 
as  in  figure  a  or  figure  b.  This  compels  the  child  to  move  one 
piece  about  in  such  a  way  that  his  getting  the  pieces  in  the. 
right  position  immediately  would  not  be  the  result  of  accident. 
To  place  the  pieces  as  in  figure  c  would  permit  him  with  one 
movement  to  get  them  in  the  right  position  and  since  this  is 
almost  the  only  movement  which  it  is  possible  for  him  to  make 
with  them  the  final  result  might  be  merely  accidental. 

CHILDREN  OF  Six  YEARS 

I.  Distinguishes  between  morning  and  evening. — The 
question  is  asked,  "Is  it  morning  or  afternoon  now?"  This 
tests  the  child's  comprehension  of  this  simple  measure  of  time. 
The  writer  always  asks,  in  addition,  after  the  child's  answer, 
"How  do  you  know  it  is."  The  answer  to  this  question  always 
indicates  the  event  of  the  day  which  the  child  has  set  up  as 
his  means  of  measuring  time.  He  will  say,  "Because  I  have 
just  had  my  breakfast,"  or  "my  lunch,"  or  some  event  of  the 
school  day,  such  as  "We  had  our  reading."  Many  children, 
who  answer  the  question  wrong,  will,  however,  answer  properly 
such  questions  as,  "Do  you  have  breakfast  in  the  morning  or 
in  the  afternoon?"  "Do  you  have  supper  in  the  afternoon  or 
in  the  morning?"  "Do  you  go  to  school  in  the  morning  or  in 
the  afternoon?",  and  so  on.  This  further  precautionary  ques- 
tioning shows  whether  or  not  the  child  has  set  up  any  type  of 
time  measure,  though  he  may  have  failed  to  take  notice  of  the 


22  CLARA  SCHMITT 

particular  event  of  the  day  at  the  time  of  his  examination,  which 
divides  morning  and  afternoon  for  him. 

Binet  says  of  this  test — "One  expects,  we,  ourselves,  expected 
more  brilliant  results.  We  would  have  judged  that  children 
could  distinguish  between  morning  and  afternoon  long  before 
the  age  of  six.  It  is  a  distinction  which  appears  so  easy.  Think 
of  the  fact  that  six-year  old  children  are  the  oldest  in  the 
'maternelle'  schools.  Recall  that  the  program  of  these  schools 
provide  for  the  teaching  of  history  and  geography;  'the  prin- 
cipal irregularities  of  the  earth's  surface,  brief  biographies  from 
natural  history,'  read  the  rules  of  the  schools  'maternelle'  of 
the  department  of  the  Seine.  Is  it  not  rather  ridiculous  to 
talk  about  natural  history  to  children  who  cannot  yet  distinguish 
between  morning  and  evening." 

Bobertag  found  that  of  55  six-year  old  children  45  per  cent 
answered  the  question  correctly;  of  126  seven-year  old  children 
69  per  cent  answered  the  question  correctly. 

II.  Defines  in  terms  of  use.< — The  child  is  asked  successively 
"What  is  a  fork?  What  is  a  table?  What  is  a  chair?  What 
is  a  horse?  What  is  a  mama?"  This  tests  the  child's  ability 
to  abstract  and  put  in  language  form  certain  characteristic 
qualities  of  familiar  objects. 

Binet  finds  that  up  to  nine  years  of  age  the  majority  of  chil- 
dren define  these  objects  in  terms  of  use  only;  of  a  fork,  "It 
is  to  eat  with";  of  a  table,  "To  eat  on  and  to  put  things  on"; 
of  a  chair,  "To  sit  on";  of  a  mama,  "She  takes  care  of  the 
children."  After  nine  years  of  age  the  definitions  are  in  terms 
superior  to  use.  Of  a  fork,  "It  is  an  object  used  for  eating"; 
of  a  horse,  "It  is  an  animal";  of  a  mama,  "She  is  the  mother 
of  a  child,"  etc.  Other  definitions  superior  to  use  are  those 
which  describe,  such  as  "A  fork  has  four  prongs  and  a  handle, 
it  is  made  of  silver,"  etc.  Very  young  children  will  answer  with 
silence  or,  "A  fork  is  a  fork." 

Bobertag  points  out  that  many  children,  who  are  intelligent 
and  who  are  not  loath  to  take  the  trouble  to  think,  remain 
silent  or  say,  "I  do  not  know."  Certainly  these  children  know 
what  a  fork  is  as  well  as  the  others  who  make  some  kind  of 


STANDARDIZATION  OF  TESTS  23 

answer,  and  neither  are  they  less  experienced  in  the  use  of 
language.  They  make  the  problem  a  very  difficult  one  for 
themselves  and  are  in  a  state  of  mind  similar  to  that  of  the 
adult  if  one  suddenly  asks  him,  "What  is  a  hole?";  or  what  he 
understands  by  the  term  state  or  truth.  That  this  is  the  con- 
dition of  the  minds  of  some  children  is  shown  by  the  fact  that 
they  are  able  to  show  a  greater  intelligence  concerning  the  dis- 
tinguishing characteristics  of  the  articles  for  which  a  definition 
is  asked  if  in  the  beginning  one  guides  their  thought  in  some 
direction.  Such  guidance  may  take  the  form: — 'A  fork  is 
to—  ?",  or  "A  fork  is  of—  ?",  or  "A  fork  appears 
how —  ?".  Children  in  their  endeavor  to  find  a  good  answer 
will  sometimes  whisper  quietly  to  themselves  "It  is  a — " 
Then  they  give  up  the  problem  and  venture,  "I  do  not  know." 

The  writer  has  found  many  normal  children  who  must  be 
guided  into  an  answer  because  they  do  not  see  the  reason  for 
asking,  what  to  them,  seems  so  simple  a  question.  They  have 
just  the  attitude  of  the  adult  when  the  latter  is  suddenly  asked, 
"What  is  a  fork?"  So  many  possibilities  for  answer  crowd 
into  the  mind  and,  not  knowing  for  what  purpose  the  question 
was  asked,  they  stare  and  answer  nothing.  In  the  school  in 
which  these  tests  were  given,  the  children  of  the  second  and 
third  grades  had  studied  and  read  of  the  customs  of  foreign 
people.  It  was  found  expedient  to  guide  the  child  in  this  way : 
"You  know  in  Japan  they  do  not  have  forks.  If  you  were 
there  someone  might  ask  you — What  is  a  fork?  What  would 
you  tell  him?" 

Bobertag  maintains  further  that  one  is  not  justified  in  sub- 
ordinating the  use  definition  to  other  types  of  definitions  of  the 
object.  He  asks,  "Is  it  not  much  more  important  that  one 
should  know  of  the  fork  that  it  is  to  eat  with  than  that  it  is 
of  iron,  or  pointed,  or  has  a  steel  handle  and  two  sharp  points  ?" 
The  use  of  the  fork  for  eating  is,  he  says,  doubtless  its  most 
important  characteristic.  The  others  stand  only  in  the  relation 
of  further  information  concerning  the  thing  defined.  It  is 
probable  that  the  tendency  to  add  further  definition  to  that  of 
use  is  due,  he  thinks,  to  certain  methods  of  school  instruction. 


CLARA  SCHM1TT 


In  the  school  the  question,  "What  is  a —  ?"  is  generally 
banned.  If  a  teacher  wishes  to  learn,  for  instance  whether  the 
child  knows  what  a  revolver  is,  he  requires  of  him  when  he 
answers  not  only,  "It  is  a  weapon,  or  a  hand  weapon,"  but  he 
also  asks  him  how  a  revolver  looks,  what  one  does  with  it,  etc. 
It  therefore  follows  that  the  child  of  six  years,  who  has  had 
little  of  such  training  in  exact  expression,  will  answer  the  ques- 
tion, "What  is  a  fork?"  with  what  had  to  him  been  the  dis- 
tinguishing characteristic  of  a  fork,  and  the  most  important 
in  his  experience.  The  older  child  adds  in  addition  to  this  or 
substitutes  for  it  some  such  further  information  as  Binet 
designates,  "Definition  superior  to  use." 

That  the  definition  may  be  made  of  further  use  for  measuring 
mental  development  than  that  conceived  by  Binet  is  shown  by 
Bobertag  with  the  use  of  more  and  other  words  than  those 
used  by  Binet.  Bobertag  uses  the  following  words : — fork,  chair, 
tongs,  kitchen,  doll,  carriage,  horse,  soldier,  penny,  rose.  These 
were  selected  because — I.  They  could  be  easily  defined  by  use. 
2.  They  could  be  easily  defined  by  description.  3.  They  could 
be  easily  defined  by  means  of  classifying  concepts.  The  de- 
velopment of  the  child  from  the  use  concept  to  the  class  concept 
in  his  definition  of  words  is  shown  in  the  following  table. 


Age  of 
Child      Fork 
5  yr.    Knife 

5  yr.    To  use  with 
potatoes 
6  yr.    To  eat 
7  yr.    To  eat 

7yr.    Of  iron 

Chair 

Doll 
Frieda  has   a 
doll 
To  play  with 

To  carry 
To  play 

Of  glass 

Horse 
It  has  ears 

It  pulls 

To  run 
To  be  hitched 
to  a  wagon 

Of  flesh 

Soldier 

Credit 
given  by 
Bobertag 

+ 
+ 

+ 

To  sit  upon 

To  sit 
Something 
upon  which 
one  may  sit 
Of  wood 

A  soldier 

To  march 
He    plays 
music 

Has  a  unl- 

r  _                          « 

8yr.    A  handle  A  back  and  a    Plaything 

with  3  seat  and 

prongs  four  legs 

pyr.    A    kitchen  A  piece  of         A  plaything 

utensil  house  fur- 
niture 

loyr.    An  eating  A  piece  of        A  plaything 

utensil  furniture            for   girls 


form  and  a 
helmet  and 
a  saber 

A  back  and  a    A  man 
belly  and 
four  legs 
An  animal         A  warrior 


A   mammal 


A  protector  of 
the  father- 
land 


STANDARDIZATION  OF  TESTS  25 

III.  Copies    a    lozenge. — The    same    may    be    said    of    this 
test  as  was  said  of  the  copying  of  a  square.     The  drawing  of 
the  lines  at  other  than  right  angles  to  each  other,  and  at  just 
the  angle  to  be  an  approximately  correct  copy  of  the  model 
before  him,   may   frequently  be  more  a  test  of  motor  ability 
than   a   test  of   the   child's   intellectual   comprehension   of   the 
characteristics  of  the  model  before  him.     The  writer  has  seen 
children  with  so  little  motor  control  as  to  be  unable  to  make 
a  passable  copy,  but  who  had  an  intellectual  comprehension  of 
the  characteristics  of  the  model  such  as  to  make  them  dissatisfied 
with  their  own  copies. 

IV.  Counts  thirteen  pennies. — The  mental  abilities  underly- 
ing the  performance  of  this  test  are  the  same  as  those  under- 
lying   the    counting    of    four    pennies    discussed    above.      The 
difference   between  the  two   is   only   that   of   length   of   series. 
Whether  or  not  a  normal  child  of  six  is  able  to  count  to  thirteen 
instead  of  ten,  or  any  other  number  depends  entirely  upon  train- 
ing.    The  writer  has  found  many  children  of  kindergarten  age 
who  have  been  taught  to  count  much  more,  some  to  100. 

V.  Compares  faces  from  the  aesthetic  point  of  view. — Six 
drawings  are  used  for  this  test,  representing  heads  of  women. 
Three  are  pretty  and  three  are  ugly  or  deformed.     The  faces 
are  compared  two  at  a  time,  one  pettty  one  and  one  ugly  one, 
and  the  child  is  asked  to  tell  which  of  the  two  he  considers 
the  prettier.    This  tests  the  child's  comprehension  of  the  normal 
or  ideal  type  of  face.    That  the  faces  would  need  to  be  changed 
greatly  were  one  testing  Chinese  or  Ethiopian  children  in  their 
native  home,  goes  without  saying. 

CHILDREN  OF  SEVEN  YEARS 

I.  Right  hand,  left  ear. — The  command  is  given  the  child, 
"Show  me  your  right  hand";  and  then,  "Touch  your  left  ear." 
This  test,  at  this  early  age,  depends  upon  teaching.  Up  to  this 
time  the  child  has  not  done  work  of  any  such  degree  of  manual 
skill  as  to  bring  out  the  distinction  between  the  two  hands. 
When  he  learns  to  write  in  the  school  such  distinction  is  made. 
The  test,  if  not  passed,  may  mean  only  that  the  child  has  not 


26  CLARA  SCHMITT 

had  such  experiences  as  would  lead  him  to  distinguish  between 
right  and  left.  The  writer's  memory  of  the  learning  of  this 
distinction  may  serve  to  illustrate.  The  knowledge  was  gained 
through  the  hand-shaking  situation.  The  extending  of  the  left 
hand  was  always  inhibited  by  the  parent's  injunction,  "No, 
give  the  right  hand."  Later  in  life,  when  there  came  the 
necessity  for  distinguishing  between  the  right  and  left  hands, 
it  was  always  necessary  to  call  to  mind  the  hand-shaking  situa- 
tion, and  the  kinaesthetic  image  in  the  right  arm  which  always 
came  with  it,  served  to  distinguish  the  right  from  the  left 
arm  from  which  there  came  no  such  image.  This  method  has 
sometimes  still  to  be  resorted  to  in  order  to  distinguish  the  right 
from  the  left  hand  in  unaccustomed  situations. 

Binet  directs  in  his  grading  of  this  test  that  the  child  who 
hesitates  be  considered  a  failure  in  the  test.  But  the  child 
who  hesitates  for  a  moment  and  then  performs  the  test  correctly 
may  be  in  some  such  situation  as  regards  his  knowledge  of  right 
and  left  hand  as  has  just  been  indicated  above;  and  this  hesita- 
tion may  in  itself  be  proof  that  he  has  a  control  of  the  mental 
process  which  helps  him  to  make  the  desired  distinction. 

II.  Describes  a  picture.* — For  this  test  a  picture  is  shown  the 
child,  and  he  is  asked  to  tell  what  he  sees  in  it.  He  passes  the 
test  if  he  does  more  than  merely  enumerate  the  objects  which 
the  picture  contains.  If  he  says,  for  instance,  "A  man  and  a 
boy  are  pulling  a  cart,"  and  not  merely,  "There  is  a  man,  a 
boy  and  a  cart,"  he  has  satisfactorily  passed  the  test. 

Binet  finds  that  three  intellectual  levels  may  find  expression 
through  this  test.  The  first  occurs  at  the  age  of  three  when 
the  child  enumerates  separately  the  persons  and  objects  which 
he  sees  in  the  picture,  without  establishing  any  connection  be- 
tween them.  He  says,  "At  three  years  one  is  at  the  stage  of 
recognition,  or  identification  of  objects;  this  is  the  important, 
fundamental  work  in  the  perception  of  the  external  world  in 
comparison  with  which  all  the  other  processes  of  perception 
are  complementary."  The  second  level  is  that  of  description. 
This  is  the  level  of  seven  years.  The  third  level  is  that  of 
interpretation.  "The  meaning  of  the  picture  or  the  nature  of  the 


STANDARDIZATION  OF  TESTS  27 

people  is  told  either  by  a  brief  word  or  by  an  explanatory  remark, 
and  often  there  is  even  an  emotional  note  of  sadness  or  of 
sympathy;  it  is  possible  that  this  emotional  note  exists  with 
children  who  make  a  more  simple  response  but  they  are  unable 
to  express  it.  We  call  these  emotional  responses  interpretations, 
because  they  go  beyond  the  visual  impression,  there  is  a  real 
effort  to  explain  the  situation  depicted." 

Bobertag  criticises  Binet  on  two  counts;  i.  That  pictures  from 
which  little  in  the  way  of  description  or  interpretation  could 
be  given  were  used  by  Binet.  The  pictures,  he  thinks,  were  too 
wooden  in  character,  too  lacking  in  action.  2.  That  Binet's 
method  of  gaining  response  is  too  indefinite.  It  leaves  the  child 
in  doubt  and  in  a  vague  frame  of  mind  as  to  what  is  wanted. 
Bobertag  would  add  such  questions  as,  "What  are  the  people 
doing  here  ?  What  is  happening  here  ?  Why  is  this  one  doing 
so?"  etc.  In  this  way  he  would  seek  to  guide  the  child  into 
fruitful  channels  of  response.  By  such  methods  he  finds  numer- 
ous grades  of  individual  development  instead  of  the  three 
enumerated  by  Binet,  but,  in  general,  the  three  of  Binet. 

Bobertag,  with  his  method  of  questioning,  finds  that  children 
younger  than  fifteen,  the  position  of  the  test  for  interpretation 
in  the  1911  series,  are  able  to  make  interpretations  of  pictures. 
The  interpretations  may,  however,  not  be  correct. 

A  variation  of  this  test  was  used  by  Squire  (16).  She  gave 
five  pictures  by  noted  artists,  In  Disgrace,  by  Sigsbeeker;  In 
Summer,  by  Van  der  Veer,  Children  of  the  Press,  by  Thomp- 
son, The  Goose  Girl,  by  Millet  and  Embers  by  Eastman  John- 
son, to  children  between  the  ages  of  six  and  thirteen.  The 
pictures  were  shown  the  child  and  he  was  required  to  give  a 
name  to  each  one,  which  he  considered  appropriate  to  the  pic- 
torial representation.  There  were  ten  children  in  each  group. 
She  concludes,  "From  these  results  it  seems  fair  to  say  that: 
(i)  No  six-year  old  child  can  be  expected  completely  to  com- 
prehend a  situation  presented  pictorially.  (2)  Neither  can  a 
seven-year  old  child  be  expected  to  give  an  adequate  title — a 
child  of  this  age  seems  most  interested  in  the  appearance  of 
the  objects  presented.  (3)  The  eight-year  old  children  are  in- 


28  CLARA  SCHMITT 

clined  to  interpret  meaning  in  terms  of  action,  and  a  few  are 
able  to  give  superficial  titles.  (4)  In  the  ninth  and  tenth  years, 
while  descriptive  phases  and  activities  of  the  object  are  most 
likely  to  be  considered,  there  is,  in  the  case  of  the  first  picture, 
complete  comprehension  of  the  artist's  meaning.  The  descrip- 
tive titles,  when  given,  are  condensed  into  terse  phrases,  and 
no  longer  stretched  out  into  disjointed  sentences.  (5)  In  the 
eleventh  year  the  answers  show  a  wide  distribution,  due  mainly 
to  the  fact  that  the  proportion  of  retarded  pupils  was  greater 
in  this  year  than  any  other.  (6)  In  the  twelfth  year  the 
majority  of  names  given  to  the  pictures  would  pass  for  titles, 
although  a  large  proportion  of  them  deal  with  superficial  aspects. 
(7)  There  were  many  cases  of  complete  comprehension  in  the 
thirteenth  year.  This  imaginative  insight  could  not  be  expected 
before  adolescence."  It  is  seen  from  this  quotation  that  Mrs. 
Squire's  results  agree  closely  with  those  of  Binet,  to  whose 
method  she  adhered  in  refraining  from  asking  the  child  stimulat- 
ing questions. 

As  Bobertag  points  out,  the  results  will  vary  greatly  with  the 
type  of  pictures  chosen  and  with  the  method  of  stimulating 
the  child  to  express  himself.  Some  of  the  Squire  pictures 
are  plainly  not  within  a  small  child's  realm  of  experience  and 
are  therefore  uninterpretable  by  him.  This  is  certainly  true  of 
Children  of  the  Press,  a  crowd  of  poorly  clad  children  receiving 
papers  for  distribution,  and  Embers,  an  old  man  seated  before 
a  grate  in  which  the  fire  is  slowly  dying.  Also  The  Goose  Girl 
could  have  no  associations  with  the  experiences  of  a  young 
American  child  not  old  enough  to  have  read  of  foreign  customs. 
Mrs.  Squire  found  that  the  significance  of  In  Disgrace,  a  picture 
of  a  pouting  child  with  face  in  the  corner,  was  grasped  earlier 
and  more  frequently  than  that  of  any  other  picture.  This 
picture  certainly  portrays  one  of  the  child's  earliest  and  most 
significant  and,  perhaps,  most  vivid  experiences. 

Bobertag  selected  his  pictures  carefully  with  reference  to  the 
experiences  of  a  small  child,  and  for  this  reason  as  well  as  for 
the  more  stimulating  method  of  presentation  obtained  a  result 
which  would  lead  to  a  more  optimistic  judgment  of  the  child's 


STANDARDIZATION  OF  TESTS  29 

ability  to  interpret  or  abstract  the  meaning  of  a  situation  .from 
the  signs  by  which  it  is  portrayed. 

In  this  as  well  as  in  any  others  testing  the  child's  ability  to 
make  right  deductions  in  a  given  situation  one  must,  as  Bobertag 
pointed  out,  choose  the  conditions  of  the  situation  with  refer- 
ence to  the  child's  experience.  To  make  the  sweeping  declara- 
tion that  a  child  of  a  certain  age  does  not  reason,  from  certain 
tests  given  him  would  probably  quite  misstate  the  case.  He  may 
reason  or  abstract  correctly  within  the  limits  of  his  experience; 
or  when  the  right  motive  for  expressing  his  thought  is  supplied, 
as  is  the  case  in  the  picture  test  when  he  is  stimulated  by 
questions. 

III.  Executes  three  commissions. — The  child  is  asked  to  listen 
while  he  is  told  to  do  something,  and  then  the  instruction  is 
given  somewhat  as  follows;  "You  see  the  door  and  the  pencil 
and  the  watch;  go  close  the  door,  put  the  pencil  at  the  end  of 
the  table,  and  hand  me  the  watch."  This  is  a  test  of  the  child's 
ability  to  attend  to  a  set  of  directions  which  have  only  a  sequen- 
tial relationship  and  translate  them  into  activity.  The  test, 
according  to  Binet,  is  passed  only  if  the  child  carries  out  the 
directions  without  any  further  encouragement  such  as,  "And 
what  else?";  You  have  forgotten  something." 

The  writer  has  found  many  sluggish  and  unresponsive  chil- 
dren who  had  to  be  encouraged  in  this  way  for  one  or  two 
sets  of  directions,  but  who  then  would  follow  out  other  similar 
sets  without  this  stimulus.  One  would  certainly  deceive  him- 
self and  do  the  child  an  injustice  to  grade  him  as  a  failure  in 
this  test  without  first  arousing  his  enthusiasm  and  consequent 
response  in  some  such  way  as  has  been  indicated.  Children 
of  the  first  grade  fail  in  some  cases  in  proper  response  to  the 
school  situation, — they  will  not  attempt  to  carry  out  the  direc- 
tions of  the  teacher  in  games  and  other  schoolroom  activities, 
such  as  counting,  writing,  etc.  The  attitude  of  the  teacher  to- 
ward such  a  child  and  her  further  educational  procedure  with 
him  is  wholly  determined  by  the  judgment  which  she  forms  of 
his  case.  She  must  know  whether  he  is  by  reason  of  innate 
mental  defect  incapable  of  such  response,  or  whether  his  failure 


30  CLARA  SCHMITT 

is  due  to  some  other  factor  of  disposition,  emotion,  will,  or 
interest.  The  type  of  stimulus  which  Binet  inhibits  is  necessary 
to  show  to  what  one  may  attribute  such  failure. 

IV.  Counts  nine  sous. — This  test  has  for  its  material  three 
objects  of  a  value  of  one  each,  and  three  of  a  value  of  two 
each.     Binet  used  pieces  of  French  money.     Dr.  Goddard  uses 
postage   stamps,   and  the  writer  uses   small   squares   of   paper 
marked  "i,  i,"  etc.,  since  the  numbers  on  the  postage  stamp 
are  not  easily  discernible.     The  test  is  one  of  the  child's  ability 
to  relate  the  symbols  of  number  to  the  idea  of  number.     There 
is  involved  also  the  idea  of  relative  value,  the  value  of  one 
thing  in  terms  of  another. 

The  idea  of  relative  value  involved  in  buying  and  selling  is 
one  which  first  appears  at  some  time  between  five  and  seven 
years. 

If  so  much  of  number  work  as  this  test  involves  is  taught 
in  the  first  grade,  the  majority  of  children  seven  years  of  age 
will  be  able  to  pass  it.  If  number  work  is  delayed  until  the 
second  grade,  as  is  the  case  in  some  schools,  many  children 
seven  years  of  age  will  be  unable  to  pass  the  test.  The  use 
of  tests  similar  to  this  is  discussed  in  a  later  chapter. 

V.  Names  four  colors. — This  tests  the  child's  ability  to  ab- 
stract a  quality  and  name  it.    The  colors  red,  green,  yellow  and 
blue  are  to  be  recognized  without  error.     The  writer  finds  no 
such  refinement  of  method  as  is  insisted  upon  by  Wallin  (17) 
necessary.     He  directs  that  saturated  colors  and  dull,  not  shiny, 
surfaces  be  used  in  the  test.     If  a  child  knows  red  as  a  quality 
he  knows  it  whether  associated  with  a  dull  or  a  shiny  surface. 
If  the  character  of  the  article  whose  color  he  is  to  name  inter- 
feres with  his  recognition  of  the  color — though  the  writer  has 
never  found  so  anomolous  a  case, — it  would  certainly  be  proof 
that  he  did  not  know  the  color. 

In  order  to  relieve  the  situation  and  test  of  any  air  of  formal- 
ity— which  is  always  a  desirable  thing  to  accomplish  in  an 
examination — the  writer  is  accustomed  to  ask  the  colors  of  any 
objects  at  hand  which  happen  to  be  of  the  required  color 
sufficiently  saturated  and  in  sufficiently  large  masses  of  solid 


STANDARDIZATION  OF  TESTS  31 

color,  such  as  the  color  of  the  book  lying  before  him,  the 
pencil  which  he  is  using,  etc.  The  writer  finds  this  test  possible 
for  children  younger  than  seven.  The  ability  to  distinguish 
colors  exists  earlier  than  the  age  of  seven,  but  its  expression 
depends  upon  training.  This  is  shown  by  the  fact  that  kinder- 
garten children  are  trained  in  the  recognition  of  colors,  and 
normal  children  of  kindergarten  experience  know  them. 

As  was  pointed  out  by  Binet,  the  naming  of  colors  is  a  step 
in  advance  of  the  discrimination  of  them.  Many  defective 
children  can  not  name  colors  correctly  but  can  correctly-  sort 
color  cards. 

CHILDREN  OF  EIGHT  YEARS 

i.  Compares  two  remembered  objects. — Binet  says  of  this 
test:  "This  is  a  valuable  test  because  it  does  not  depend  in 
the  least  on  instruction,  and  brings  into  play  the  natural  good 
sense  of  the  subject.  It  consists  in  investigating  whether  the 
subject  can,  in  thinking  of  two  objects,  distinguish  a  difference 
between  them;  the  perception  of  the  difference  is,  in  fact,  the 
habitual  and  most  natural  result  of  the  comparison." 

In  this  test  one  says  to  the  child :  "You  have  seen  a  butter- 
fly, have  you  not?,  and  you  have  seen  flies.  Tell  me  how  is  it 
that  they  are  not  alike.  How  do  you  know  them  apart  when 
you  see  them  at  the  same  time?"  In  the  same  way  he  is  asked 
to  tell  the  difference  between  wood  and  glass,  and  paper  and 
cloth.  The  child  is  expected  to  give  what  constitutes  some 
significant  difference  between  the  two  objects,  such  as,  "The 
butterfly  is  larger  than  the  fly" ;  or,  "has  brightly  colored  wings 
and  the  fly  has  not."  The  types  of  response  observed  to  this 
test  by  Binet,  Bobertag,  and  others,  are:  I.  The  child  main- 
tains silence.  2.  He  gives  an  answer  which  involves  no  dis- 
crimination, "Because  they  are  different,"  or  "Because  it  is  a 
fly  and  one  is  a  butterfly,"  or  "They  have  wings."  3.  He 
gives  some  non-discriminating  differences  such  as  "Wood  is 
thick  and  glass  is  thin,"  or  "Paper  is  whiter  than  cloth." 
4.  He  gives  a  correct  answer  for  the  first  pair  asked  and,  find- 
ing that  answer  receives  approval,  uses  it  for  the  other  pairs 


32  CLARA  SCHMITT 

and  cannot  be  induced  to  make  the  correct  discrimination  for 
any  others.  5.  He  gives  a  correct  and  discriminative  response. 

Bobertag  adds  to  this  test  one  which  requires  the  child  to  give 
the  characteristics  in  which  two  objects  are  alike.  He  uses 
sun  and  moon;  glass  and  ice;  honey  and  glue.  He  says  to 
the  child:  "The  sun  and  the  moon  are  alike  in  something,  are 
they  not?  How  is  it  that  they  are  alike?"  The  correct  answer 
is  to  the  effect  that  they  are  both  round,  or  that  they  both 
shine,  etc.  Bobertag  finds  that  the  test  for  differences  and  the 
test  for  likenesses  show  that  the  two  abilities  differ  in  many 
cases.  Some  children  are  able  to  pass  the  one  and  not  the 
other. 

II.  Counts  backward  to  i  from  20. — In  this  test  the  child 
is  asked  to  count  from  one  to  twenty,  and  then  he  is  asked 
to  count  backward  from  twenty  to  one.  The  test  is  one  of 
the  child's  ability  to  rearrange  mental  content  in  a  new  and 
prescribed  way.  It  shows  that  he  is  able  to  control  his  associa- 
tions in  such  a  way  as  to  produce  a  desired  result.  Reciting  the 
months  of  the  year  and  the  alphabet  backwards  are  analogous 
and  perhaps  more  difficult  tests.  The  difficulty  of  such  a  test 
depends  largely  upon  the  familiarity  with  and  length  of  series. 

Binet  considers  the  test  passed  if  the  child  takes  not  more 
than  twenty  seconds  for  the  process.  In  the  opinion  of  the 
writer  the  time  required  should  not  be  so  rigidly  dictated.  Binet 
also  considers  the  test  a  failure  if  the  child  can  be  detected 
counting  forward  up  to  the  desired  point  in  order  to  get  the 
next  item  of  the  reconstructed  series.  The  writer's  introspection 
at  doing  this  sort  of  thing  shows  that  there  is  no  other  method 
of  doing  it,  unless  the  series  is  so  familiar  as  to  make  this 
method  of  getting  the  next  desired  item  unnecessary,  or  as  to 
so  shorten  the  process  as  to  make  it  seem  to  be  altogether 
eliminated.  With  the  writer  the  alphabet  is  not  so  familiar 
a  series  as  to  make  this  method,  when  repeating  it  backwards, 
unnecessary.  The  child  who  is  able  to  hit  upon  such  a  method 
of  doing  his  work  and  to  keep  his  mind  to  the  task  so  that  he 
makes  no  errors;  who  does  not  forget  that  his  task  is  to  count 
backward  and  to  inhibit  the  counting  forward  association,  shows 


STANDARDIZATION  OF  TESTS  33 

his  ability  to  control  his  mental  processes,  and  has  surely  passed 
the  test. 

Defective  children  of  this  age  are  either  unable  to  understand 
what  is  required  of  them,  and  reply  with  absolute  silence,  or 
can  find  no  method  of  doing  the  work  even  though  they  do 
understand  what  is  required  and  make  a  valiant  effort.  Some 
defective  children  who  do  understand  what  is  required  and  who 
have  a  method  for  doing  it,  are  unable  to  inhibit  the  usual  count- 
ing forward  association,  and  after  one  or  two  successes  at  get- 
ting the  desired  items  for  the  new  series,  begin  again  to  count 
forward.  The  performance  in  such  case  becomes  as  follows : 
nineteen,  eighteen,  seventeen,  sixteen,  seventeen,  eighteen,  nine- 
teen, twenty-  twenty-one,  etc. 

The  test  is,  in  the  writer's  experience,  one  that  can  be  passed 
by  children  younger  than  eight  years  of  age,  in  case  the  series 
used  is  one  with  which  they  are  perfectly  familiar.  Kinder- 
garten children  who  can  count  to  ten  or  any  other  number,  can 
also  successfully  count  backward  in  the  series  with  which  they 
feel  a  perfect  'familiarity.  Some  of  the  kindergarten  children 
of  those  examined  by  the  writer,  who  could  count  to  twenty, 
failed  to  count  backward  from  twenty  but  succeeded  in  count- 
ing backward  from  ten,  because  the  series  between  ten  and 
twenty  was  not  so  familiar  to  them  as  to  have  become  automatic, 
and  they  could  not  so  divide  the  attention  between  the  task  to 
be  accomplished  and  the  imperfectly  acquired  series. 

III.  Indicates  omissions  in  pictures.    Four  pictures  are  shown, 
each  lacking  some  elemental  part  of  the  physical  make  up,  and 
the  child  is  asked  to  tell  what  is  missing  in  the  picture.     This 
tests  the  child's  ability  to  compare  the  representation  of  a  type 
with  the  type  itself;  in  this  case  the  human  body. 

IV.  Gives  day  and  date.     In  this  test  the  child  is  asked  to 
give  the  day  of  the  week,  the  month,  the  day  of  the  month, 
and  the  year.    An  error  of  three  or  four  days  is  allowed. 

That  this  test  may  be  passed  two  conditions  are  necessary. 
The  subject  must  possess  an  appreciation  of  the  conception  of 
the  measure  of  time  involved  in  the  date,  and  he  must  engage 
in  such  activities  as  make  use  of  the  date,  such  as  the  writing 


34  CLARA  SCHMITT 

of  letters  or  the  reading  of  the  daily  papers.  A  subject  may 
fail  to  pass  the  test  merely  because  he  does  not  engage  in  such 
daily  occupations  as  require  note  of  the  current  day,  though  he 
is  quite  capable  of  the  conception  of  time  measure.  On  the 
other  hand  he  may  be  able  to  recite  the  date  without  possessing 
any  idea  of  the  time  measure  for  which  it  stands.  In  the  school 
children  are  frequently  required  to  place  the  date  on  written 
work.  In  such  case  large  groups  of  children  may  know  the 
date  without  the  corresponding  idea  of  time  measure.  In  order 
to  determine  whether  the  child's  knowledge  is  only  the  result 
of  such  specific  teaching  or  whether  it  is  related  to  the  time 
conception,  the  test  may  be  extended  by  asking  the  questions, 
"What  day  of  the  week  was  yesterday?  What  will  be  to- 
morrow? What  was  last  month?  etc."  Many  defective  children 
are  able  to  recite  the  days  of  the  week,  the  months  of  the  year, 
and  give  the  date  without  being  able  to  answer  the  foregoing 
questions  correctly.  Occasionally  a  child  fails  to  pass  the  test 
according  to  the  standard  set  by  Binet,  but  is  able  to  answer 
these  questions  correctly  with  reference  to  the  date  which  he 
has  given.  Such  a  case  should  be  given  full  credit  for  the  test. 

Binet  says,  "We  found  that  in  the  schools  'maternelle,'  a 
language  lesson  is  given  every  day  at  the  opening  of  school 
in  which  the  day  and  date  are  taught.  The  children  are  told 
the  day,  date  and  year,  and  then  made  to  repeat  it.  However, 
not  one  child  in  the  school  was  able  to  give  us  the  complete 
information,  nor  one  the  name  of  the  year  alone;  and  for  the 
month  many  answers  were  given,  even  when  in  reality  it  was 
February  8.  ...  It  is  a  curious  fact  that  children  fail  most  often 
to  give  the  year.  They  give  no  year,  they  remain  silent  for 
they  do  not  know  it.  Perhaps  a  year  is  for  them  so  great  a 
lapse  of  time  that  they  can  form  no  idea  of  it." 

If  Binet's  rinding  concerning  this  test  and  the  fourth  of  the 
nine-year-old  tests  in  which  the  child  enumerates  the  months 
of  the  year  are  true  for  the  ages  under  which  they  are  put,  one 
must  conclude  that  the  child  who  knows  the  day  and  date  at 
eight  years  of  age,  but  cannot  know  the  months  until  he  is 
nine  years  of  age,  is  able  to  pass  the  former  test  only  because 


STANDARDIZATION  OF  TESTS  35 

of  specific  training.  If  he  cannot  know  the  months  of  the 
year  until  he  is  nine  years  of  age,  how  can  he  have  a  conception 
of  the  time  measure  for  which  the  date  stands? 

V.  Repeats  5  digits. — The  simplest  and  most  effective  way 
of  giving  this  test  is  as  follows: — One  says  to  the  child — 
"Listen!"  and  when  he  is  attending  "2,  7,  i,  say  it."  When 
he  has  responded  correctly  one  says  again,  merely  keeping  the 
same  attitude  of  attention  toward  the  child,  "4,  9,  6,  3,"  and 
so  on  until  he  fails.  Should  his  attention  at  any  time  wander, 
it  is  most  quickly  brought  back  by  the  short  command,  "Listen !" 

An  added  feature  of  this  test  is  to  discover  how  many  repeti- 
tions of  the  set  of  digits,  which  is  just  beyond  his  memory 
span,  is  necessary  before  he  can  learn  to  repeat  it.  If  the  child 
can  repeat  only  four  digits,  then  a  set  of  five  may  be  repeated 
again  and  again  until  he.  can  repeat  it  correctly.  This  gives 
some  indication  of  the  child's  ability  to  take  on  a  new  habit 
of  attention,  or  to  advance  to  a  higher  stage  of  ability. 

The  writer  finds  frequently  a  type  of  defective  child  who, 
when  given  the  set,  for  instance,  3,  5,  9,  i,  4,  will  repeat  all 
the  digits  but  in  some  inverted  order,  such  as  3,  9,  5,  i,  4.  One 
says  to  the  child,  "No  you  did  not  say  them  correctly.  Say 
them  just  as  I  do,"  and  they  are  repeated  for  him  again.  He, 
however,  persists  in  saying  them  in  the  order  in  which  he  first 
said  them.  It  seems,  that  having  made  one  set  of  associations 
he  is  unable  to  break  it  up  in  favor  of  another.  Upon  one 
occasion  a  teacher,  hearing  this  type  of  response,  said  of  the 
child  "That's  just  the  way  he  is  in  reading.  If  he  ever  pro- 
nounces a  word  wrong,  it  is  very  difficult  to  get  him  to  give 
the  correct  pronunciation."  The  writer  has  observed,  in  addi- 
tion, the  following  types  of  response  on  the  part  of  defective 
children,  i.  They  maintain  silence.  2.  After  the  child's  limen 
is  passed,  he  repeats  only  the  last  one  or  two  digits  of  the  set 
given  him.  3.  He  repeats  a  set  of  numbers  which  may  not 
be  at  all  those  given  him.  4.  He  starts  out  with  one  of  the 
numbers  which  has  been  given  him,  the  first  or  the  last  perhaps, 
and  then  goes  on  counting  in  serial  order. 

Bobertag  has  found  that  children  of  five  years  of  age  can 


36  CLARA  SCHMITT 

reproduce  a  group  of  four  digits;  at  seven  years  of  age  a  group 
of  five ;  and  at  ten  years  of  age  a  group  of  six.  He  adds  to  the 
test,  after  the  child's  response,  "Was  it  right?"  to  which  the  child 
answers  3;^  or  no.  He  finds  that  feeble-minded  children  main- 
tain that  what  they  have  said  is  quite  right,  even  though  quite 
unlike  the  set  given  them.  The  normal  child  is  more  likely  to 
say,  "I  do  not  know,"  or  "It  may  be  wrong." 

CHILDREN  OF  NINE  YEARS 

I.  Gives  change  from  twenty  sous. — This  test  depends  both 
upon  ability,  teaching  and  experience.  The  writer  has  not  found 
the  elaborate  refinement  of  method  described  by  Binet  for  this 
test  a  necessary  condition  for  its  proper  performance.  It  is 
quite  sufficient  to  ask  the  child  to  tell  what  would  be  the  change 
that  one  would  receive,  and  then,  if  he  has  answered  correctly, 
to  tell  in  what  kind  of  pieces  one  might  receive  it.  When  a 
quarter  is  used,  and  the  amount  purchased  is  four  cents,  the 
child  who  answers  correctly  does  not  fail  to  tell  you  that  your 
change  might  be  in  the  form  of  two  dimes  and  a  penny.  As 
a  test  of  mathematical  knowledge  this  one  does  not  test  the 
maximum  mathematical  ability  of  the  child  at  this  age,  since 
the  school  requires  more  complex  problems  of  him  in  the  grade 
in  which  he  normally  belongs  at  nine  years  of  age  than  the  test 
implies.  It  is  a  test  in  which  experience  of  a  specific  sort  enters 
very  largely  in  determining  the  type  of  performance.  Many 
children  who  fail  in  doing  the  mathematical  work  of  the  school, 
but  who  are  permitted  to  use  money,  are  able  to  make  change 
so  far  as  their  specific  experiences  with  money  permit  them  to 
do  so.  A  thirteen-year-old  defective  boy  in  the  third  grade  in 
school  was  unable  to  do  the  arithmetic  work  of  the  third  grade. 
He  could  not  learn  to  subtract  or  multiply.  He,  however,  could 
make  change  with  larger  denominations  and  in  more  complex 
situations  than  this  test  calls  for.  He  had  learned  to  do  this 
through  collecting  fares  in  the  cab  which  he  drove  from  his 
father's  small  hotel  to  the  railway  station.  He  could  tell  how 
much  thirteen  twenty-five  cent  fares  amounted  to,  and  yet  was 
unable  to  so  generalize  his  mathematical  experience  as  to  be 


STANDARDIZATION  OF  TESTS  37 

able  to  work  out  this  or  other  problems  with  which  he  had  no 
experience,  by  the  use  of  the  mathematical  processes  taught  in 
his  grade.  The  children  of  his  grade  can  work  out  problems 
with  which  they  have  had  no  specific  experience.  They  can 
apply  the  arithmetic  of  the  school  for  the  purpose. 

II.  Defines    in    terms    superior    to    use. — Discussed    above. 

III.  Recognises  all  the  pieces  of  our  money. — This  test  is, 
perhaps,  in  its  implications  of  innate  ability  not  different  from 
that  one  in  the  fourth  year  list  in  which  the  child  is  asked  to 
name  different  objects.     To  be  sure,  the  distinguishing  differ- 
ences between  two  pieces  of  money  may  be  very  much  finer  than 
the  differences  between  a  key,  a  knife,  and  a  penny.    Whether  or 
not  the  child  at  this  age  knows  these  particular  pieces  of  money 
depends  upon  the  same  ability  which  enables  him  at  four  years 
to  name  objects  of  any  other  kind,  plus  the  specific  experience 
which  enables  him  to  name  different  coins. 

The  writer  has  not  found  it  necessary  to  show  the  child  all 
these  pieces  of  money  in  order  to  determine  whether  or  not 
he  is  able  to  recognize  them.  The  child  who  can  recognize 
them  is  able  to  tell  you  in  what  respect  a  five  dollar  bill  differs 
from  a  one  dollar  bill,  or  a  silver  dollar  from  a  half-dollar, 
sufficiently  well  to  show  his  acquaintance  with  them. 

IV.  Enumerates  the  months. — The  ability  which  underlies  this 
test  is  not  different  from  that  which  enables  the  child  at  an 
earlier  age  to  learn  the  counting  series.    The  difference  is  merely 
one  of  specific  instruction,  plus  whatever  difference  there  may 
be  in  the  difficulty  of  learning  the  two  series.     Whether  or  not 
the  majority  of  children  are  able  to  repeat  the  months  of  the 
year   at   this   age   depends  upon   the  school   curriculum.     The 
table  of  time  measure  is  taught  in  that  part  of  the  arithmetical 
course  which  takes  up  other  tables  of  measurement.     In  the 
Chicago  schools  this  occurs  in  the  fourth  grade.     Children  who 
begin  school  at  six  years  of  age  and  progress  normally,  one 
grade  every  year,  are  in  the  fourth  grade  at  nine  years  of  age. 
It  is  probable,  since  much  use  is  made  of  the  date  before  this 
grade,  that  the  names  of  the  months  of  the  year  have  been  learned 
before  this  time.    At  any  rate,  the  majority  of  children  nine  years 


38  CLARA  SCHMITT 

of  age  during  the  first  half  of  the  year  in  the  fourth  grade  learn 
the  various  tables  of  time  measure,  including  the  months  of 
the  year,  if  they  have  not  previously  learned  them.  If  this  specific 
bit  of  instruction  came  at  an  earlier  or  later  period  than  this 
in  the  school,  it  would  not,  of  course,  be  a  suitable  nine-year-old 
test ;  and  for  that  reason  it  cannot  be  considered  a  test  of  innate 
ability  alone.  The  most  important  consideration  with  this  test 
is  the  conception  of  time  involved. 

It  is  characteristic  of  defective  children,  who  can  repeat  the 
months  of  the  year,  that  they  cannot  do  it  upon  the  demand, 
"Say  for  me  the  names  of  the  months  of  the  year."  They  re- 
main silent,  not  knowing  what  is  wanted.  If  one  starts  them 
out,  however,  with,  "January,  February,  go  on  now,  say  them 
for  me,"  they  can  begin  and  repeat  them  correctly.  In  this  case 
they  have  been  able  to  learn  a  series,  and  when  it  is  started  for 
them  they  can  go  on  with  all  of  its  terms  but  have  been  unable 
to  relate  the  series  to  another  conception.  They  have  no  idea 
of  the  meaning  of  the  series  as  a  measure  of  time. 

V.  Understands  easy  questions. — The  questions  are,  (i) 
"What  would  you  do  if  you  missed  a  train?"  (2)  "What 
would  you  do  if  one  of  your  playmates  should  hit  you  without 
meaning  to  do  so?"  (3)  "What  would  you  do  if  you  broke 
something  belonging  to  someone  else?"  The  answer  to  the 
first  one  of  these  questions  depends  upon  the  specific  experience 
of  the  child  in  this  particular  situation.  Binet  considers  the 
answer  to  this  question,  "Go  home  again,"  as  incorrect;  but 
in  many  instances  this  is  what  is  done.  Just  what  one  would 
do  of  course  depends  upon  circumstances.  The  child  whose 
family  would  have  small  choice  of  trains,  say  only  one  a  day, 
has  observed  that  they  do  go  home  again  if  the  train  is  missed. 
Question  number  two  shows  the  child's  understanding  of  the 
relation  of  conduct  to  motive.  Question  number  three  shows 
his  understanding  of  the  accepted  moral  way  of  meeting  the 
situation. 


STANDARDIZATION  OF  TESTS 


39 


CHILDREN  OF  TEN  YEARS 

I.  Arranges  five  weights. — The  weights  are  in  the  form  of 
boxes  or  blocks  of  identical  size  and  color  weighing  respectively 
three,  six,  nine,  twelve  and  fifteen  grams.  The  child  is  asked 
to  arrange  them  in  the  order  of  weight.  This  tests,  in  addition 
to  the  ability  underlying  the  weight  test  in  the  five-year  series, 
the  child's  ability  to  distinguish  small  differences  in  weight.  In 
the  writer's  opinion,  the  grasp  of  the  idea  of  arranging  them 
serially,  and  an  intelligent  attempt  to  do  so,  is  the  significant 
part  of  the  test.  An  error  in  the  arrangement,  such  as  fifteen, 
twelve,  nine,  three,  six,  is  of  little  or  no  importance  in  judging 
his  general  intelligence. 

This  test  was  given  by  the  writer  to  a  college  class  of  twenty 
students.  Each  person  was  given  all  the  time  that  he  desired 
to  arrange  the  weights  to  his  satisfaction,  and  was  permitted 
to  lift  and  test  them  in  his  own  way  over  and  over  until  he  was 
satisfied  that  he  had  arranged  them  correctly  in  the  order  of 
weight,  from  the  heaviest  to  the  lightest.  Of  these  twenty 
college  students,  ten  arrived  at  a  correct  result  and  ten  of  them 
did  not.  The  ten  who  failed  had  some  such  error  as  is  indicated 
above. 


II.  Copies  drawings  from  memory. — The  child  is  asked  to  re- 
produce from  memory  two  drawings  after  being  allowed  to 
look  at  them  for  ten  seconds.  One  says  to  him,  "I  shall  now 
show  you  two  little  drawings  which  you  may  look  at  for  only 
a  little  while.  When  I  take  the  drawings  away,  then  you  are 
to  draw  them  as  well  as  you  can  remember.  As  you  have  only 
a  few  seconds  to  look  at  them,  you  must  be  careful  to  look  at 
both  of  the  drawings."  When  the  child  is  ready  with  his  arms 
and  pencil  in  position  and  attention  alert,  the  drawings  are 


40  CLARA  SCHMITT 

exposed  for  ten  seconds.  This  test  is  one  of  a  particular  type 
of  memory, — the  visual.  Psychological  investigations  of  dif- 
ferent types  of  memory,  visual,  auditory,  etc.,  go  to  show  that 
they  vary  in  ability  with  the  individual.  It  is  reasonable  to 
suppose  that  children  of  the  same  degree  of  general  intelligence 
might  vary  considerably  in  their  ability  to  pass  such  a  test  as 
this,  unless  the  drawings  are  so  simple  that  they  come  within 
the  lowest  range  of  ability  at  visual  memory,  or  the  time  of 
exposure  so  long  as  to  place  them  within  the  lowest  ability  at 
learning  the  drawings.  This  learning  may  consist  in  getting 
a  very  thoroughly  stamped  visual  image;  or  it  may  consist  in 
transferring  the  visual  imagery  into  terms  of  other  imagery; 
or  the  visual  imagery  may  be  partially  aided  or  propped  up, 
so  to  speak,  by  a  partial  transference  into  other  types  of  imagery. 
Many  intelligent  children  show  that  they  have  a  method  of 
aiding  the  visual  memory.  Sometimes  it  is  with  a  verbal 
analysis  of  the  drawings  before  them.  They  will  say  softly 
to  themselves  of  the  second  one,  "three  squares  in  a  row"  and 
then  proceed  to  draw  the  figure  more  or  less  accurately,  often 
with  the  right  hand  square  turned  outward  or  the  middle  por- 
tion closed.  Of  the  first  figure  one  often  has  evidence  in  the 
result  that  the  drawing  has  been  interpreted.  The  child  says 
softly  "a  box,"  and  then  reproduces  the  figure,  sometimes  cor- 
rectly. Often  the  figure  is  reproduced  as  the  conventionalized 
box,  which  shows  even  if  one  did  not  hear  the  child  pronounce 
the  word,  that  he  has  so  interpreted  the  figure,  forgotten  the 
figure  itself,  and  produced  his  interpretation. 

A  clear  example  of  the  necessity  for  aiding  the  visual  imagery 
with  a  verbal  analysis  was  shown  by  a  boy  of  thirteen.  The 
test  was  given  him  in  a  way  different  from  that  prescribed  by 
Binet.  One  figure  at  a  time  was  exposed  for  three  seconds  and 
he  was  required  to  reproduce  it.  If  wrong,  it  was  exposed  again 
until  such  time  as  it  was  reproduced  correctly.  Of  the  second 
figure,  he  said  softly  to  himself,  "three  squares  in  a  row,"  and 
reproduced  the  figure  correctly,  except  that  the  center  portion 
was  drawn  as  a  closed  square.  He  was  told  that  he  had  re- 
membered it  wrong  and  might  be  permitted  to  look  again.  With 


STANDARDIZATION  OF  TESTS  41 

the  second  exposure,  he  said,  "the  center  one  is  open,"  and 
then  reproduced  the  figure  correctly.  When  shown  the  first 
figure  he  said,  ''two  squares,"  and  reproduced  the  figure  with 
the  enclosed  square  in  the  exact  center  of  the  outer  one.  Upon 
the  second  exposure  he  reproduced  it  correctly.  When  asked 
what  he  thought  about  it  then,  he  said,  "I  said  to  myself  the 
middle  one  is  nearer  one  side." 

Many  children  aid  the  visual  memory  by  outlining  the  figure 
in  the  air  while  observing  it.  In  the  writer's  experience,  the 
defective  child  never  learns  consciously  to  help  out  his  defective 
memory  with  such  an  analysis.  He  may  or  may  not  learn  to 
represent  the  figures  correctly,  after  an  indefinite  number  of 
exposures,  but  there  never  is  evidence  that  he  finds  another  type 
of  mental  imagery  to  aid  the  visual. 

III.  Criticises  absurd  phrases. — The  absurdities  are :  ( i )  An 
unfortunate  bicycle  rider  fell  on  his  head  and  was  killed  in- 
stantly. He  was  taken  to  a  hospital,  and  they  fear  he  will 
not  recover.  (2)  I  have  three  brothers,  Paul,  Earnest,  and 
myself.  (3)  I  am  taller  than  John,  John  is  taller  than  Henry, 
and  Henry  is  taller  than  I  am.  (4)  There  was  a  railroad 
accident  yesterday,  but  it  was  not  a  bad  one;  the  number  of 
dead  is  only  forty-eight.  (5)  Some  one  said,  "If  I  should  ever 
grow  desperate  and  kill  myself,  I  would  not  choose  Friday,  be- 
cause Friday  is  an  unlucky  day  and  might  bring  me  unhappiness." 

The  performance  of  this  test  requires  the  ability  to  hold  in 
attention  the  several  elements  of  the  verbally  presented  situation, 
and  to  form  a  judgment  as  to  the  possibility  of  their  simul- 
taneous presence  in  the  situation.  The  writer's  experience  with 
this  test,  as  may  be  seen  in  the  tables  II,  III,  IV  following, 
indicates  that  children  younger  than  ten  years  of  age  are  able 
to  do  this.  To  determine  this  fact,  care  should  be  taken,  in 
giving  the  test,  if  the  child  answers  incorrectly,  to  find  if  he 
is  unable  to  hold  the  various  elements  of  the  situation  in  mind 
sufficiently  well  to  form  a  judgment,  or  if  he  has  forgotten  or 
failed  entirely  to  notice  some  of  the  elements  of  the  situation 
as  presented  to  him.  It  is  the  practice  of  the  writer  when  a 
wrong  answer  is  given  to  ask  the  child  to  repeat  the  thing  that 


42  CLARA  SCHMITT 

was  said.  Frequently  he  has  failed  to  take  note  of  some  of 
the  elements  of  the  situation;  for  instance,  in  the  first  one 
he  sometimes  has  forgotten  immediately  that  the  unfortunate 
bicycle  rider  was  killed  instantly,  in  which  case  he  says  that 
there  is  nothing  wrong  with  the  statement.  It  is  then  repeated 
for  him  and  he  is  asked  to  repeat  it  until  he  can  do  so  cor- 
rectly. It  is  only  then  that  a  wrong  response  can  be  attributed 
to  defect  of  judgment.  The  defective  child  may,  however, 
never  be  able  to  get  all  the  elements  of  the  situation  in  the 
field  of  attention  at  one  time.  Of  the  normal  children  tested 
none  required  more  than  a  third  repetition. 

Among  foreign  children  it  is  very  common  to  find  that  they 
make  use  of  such  an  expression  as,  "I  have  three  brothers,  Paul, 
Earnest  and  myself,"  with  correct  comprehension  of  the  mean- 
ing. Their  meaning  is,  "There  are  in  my  family  three  brothers" ; 
but  the  putting  of  the  statement  in  the  first  person  does  not 
show  a  lack  of  judgment  on  their  part.  It  is  merely  a  very 
common  misuse  of  language  on  the  part  of  foreign  speaking 
people. 

IV.  Understands  difficult  questions. — The  questions  are: 
(i)  What  should  you  do  if  you  were  delayed  in  getting 
started  to  school  and  knew  you  would  be  late?  (2)  What 
should  you  do  before  taking  part  in  an  important  affair? 
(3)  Why  is  a  bad  action  done  when  one  is  angry 
more  excusable  than  the  same  action  done  when  one  is 
not  angry?  (4)  What  would  you  do  if  you  were  asked 
your  opinion  of  someone  whom  you  did  not  know  well?  (5) 
Why  should  one  judge  a  person  by  his  acts  rather  than  by  his 
words?  These  questions  test  the  child's  ability  to  formulate 
a  rule  of  action  to  meet  a  given  situation. 

From  the  answers  one  can  often  determine  whether  the  child 
has  generalized  the  situation  or  has  in  mind  a  particular  situa- 
tion. To  question  number  one,  Binet  considers  only  the  answers, 
"I  should  have  to  hurry  or  "I  should  have  to  run,"  as  correct, 
the  idea  being  to  reduce  the  amount  of  tardiness.  However, 
the  rule  or  practice  adopted  by  the  particular  school  or  the  home 
may  determine  another  answer  which  would  be  equally  correct. 


STANDARDIZATION  OF  TESTS  43 

For  instance,  if  he  says,  "I  would  go  back  home  and  get  an 
excuse  from  my  mother,"  since  some  schools  make  this  require- 
ment. The  second  question  the  writer  finds  it  necessary  usually 
to  put  in  this  iform:  "What  ought  one  to  do  before  beginning 
a  very  important  piece  of  work  or  anything  that  is  very  im- 
portant?", since  the  word  affair  among  uneducated  classes  is 
not  understood.  Some  children  generalize  the  situation,  and 
answer  to  the  effect  that  they  would  think  or  reflect  about  it. 
Some  children  have  in  mind  particular  important  situations, 
usually  those  in  which  they  have  recently  engaged,  and  answer 
accordingly.  One  girl,  thinking  of  a  fine  piece  of  embroidery 
which  she  had  been  doing  for  days  in  the  schoolroom  for  a 
rather  important  purpose,  answered,  "Wash  your  hands."  A 
boy,  thinking  of  a  workshop  where  he  had  been  for  several 
-months  learning  a  trade,  said,  "Ask  the  boss  to  show  you  how." 
The  generalized  form  of  answer  shows  a  higher  range  of  in- 
telligence than  the  particularized  form.  Young  children  and 
defective  individuals  are,  if  they  answer  at  all  correctly,  more 
likely  to  particularize  the  situation  than  to  generalize  it. 

V.  Uses  three  given  words  in  two  sentences. — Binet  uses 
the  words,  Paris,  fortune,  stream;  Dr.  Goddard  uses  the  words, 
Philadelphia,  money  river;  The  writer  uses  the  words,  Chicago, 
money,  river.  Binet  says  of  this  test  that  it  shows  the  child's 
ability  to  invent  his  own  expression.  He  directs  that  the  child 
be  asked  to  write  the  sentence  or  sentences  which  he  makes. 
The  writer's  practice  is  to  ask  the  child  to  give  his  sentences 
orally.  With  very  young  children  the  word  sentence,  is  not 
understood,  and  the  child  is  asked  merely  to  tell  something 
about  these  three  things,  or  to  say  something  that  has  these 
three  words  in  what  he  says,  or  to  tell  a  story  about  them. 

The  success  of  the  test  with  children  of  different  ages  de- 
pends upon  the  words  chosen.  When  there  is  a  failure  to  re- 
spond with  a  correct  sentence  for  the  words  Chicago,  money, 
river,  the  writer  gives  other  words,  such  as  boy,  river,  ball,  when 
the  result  is  generally  successful  with  normal  children.  This 
indicates  that  the  ability  to  invent  one's  own  expression  may  be 
something  apart  from  the  ability  to  invent  an  expression  for  a 


44  CLARA  SCHMITT 

given  set  of  words.  Success  with  certain  sets  and  failure  with 
certain  other  sets  may  indicate,  among  children  of  the  same  or 
different  ages,  differences  in  experience,  or  maturity  of  thought, 
but  one  would  have  to  examine  further  than  the  set  given  by 
Binet  before  deciding  that  the  child  lacked  the  ability  to  invent 
his  own  expression. 

Young  children  fail  to  respond  to  the  words  Chicago,  money, 
river,  because  their  experiences  with  such  generalized  ideas  as 
these  is  quite  lacking;  or  in  their  specific  experiences,  the  three 
ideas  expressed  by  these  words  may  never  have  had  any  relation 
to  each  other  and  the  child  is  therefore  unable  to  form  a  train 
of  ideas  which  would  connect  them  all.  When  he  is  given  a 
set  of  words  which  come  within  the  experience  possible  to  his 
age,  he  is  successful  in  his  response. 

Illustration  of  the  influence  of  formal  educational  experience 
was  (furnished  by  the  children  whose  records  appear  in  the 
tables  below.  In  the  local  history  which  is  taught  to  the  third 
grade,  the  Chicago  river  figures  much  in  the  development  of 
the  city  of  Chicago.  The  children  from  this  grade  gave  gen- 
erally a  sentence  which  expressed  this  historical  fact, — in  effect, 
"Chicago  has  a  river  which  cost  much  money."  The  children 
of  the  other  grades  did  not  generally  give  this  sentence. 

Another  type  of  sentence  is  one  which  is  grammatically  correct 
but  is  an  invention  merely  to  fulfill  requirements,  such  as, 
"Chicago  has  a  river,  and  also  much  money  in  its  banks."  This 
type  of  sentence  is  given  very  largely  by  the  child  who  lacks 
the  historical  teaching  just  mentioned.  A  third  type  is  non- 
sensical in  meaning,  such  as,  "Chicago  makes  more  money  than 
the  river  does,"  a  sentence  given  by  a  fifteen-year-old  defective 
girl.  Squire  (16)  gave  the  set,  boy,  river,  ball,  to  six-year-old 
children  with  the  requirement  that  they  tell  a  story.  She  obtained 
a  uniform  result  showing  that  the  child  of  this  age  is  able  to 
invent  his  own  expression. 

CHILDREN  OF  TWELVE  YEARS 

I.  Resists  suggestion. — The  material  for  this  test  as  prepared 
according  to  Binet's  suggestions,  is:  "Prepare  a  booklet  of 


STANDARDIZATION  OF  TESTS  45 

six  pages.  On  the  first  page  two  lines  are  drawn  in  ink,  A  and 
B ;  the  first,  that  is  the  one  on  the  left,  is  four  centimeters  long, 
and  the  second  five  centimeters;  they  are  placed  in  line  with 
each  other  and  one  centimeter  apart;  on  the  second  page  two 
similar  lines  are  drawn,  the  first  five  centimeters,  the  second  six ; 
on  the  third  page  the  first  line  is  six  centimeters,  and  the  second 
seven;  on  each  of  the  three  following  pages  two  lines  are  drawn 
in  the  same  positions,  but  all  are  the  same  length,  seven 
centimeters." 

In  giving  the  test  one  says  to  the  child,  "Which  is  the  longer 
of  these  two  lines?"  (showing  the  first  pair),  "and  of  these 
two?"  (showing  the  second  pair),  and  so  on.  Many  children 
attempt  to  measure  the  lines;  if  not  directly  on  the  page  itself, 
in  some  other  way,  by  placing  the  fingers  appropriately  on  the 
table  before  them.  For  this  reason  the  writer  finds  it  advan- 
tageous to  say  to  the  child,  "Which  of  these  two  lines  looks 
the  longer?"  Binet  finds  that  children  under  twelve  years  of 
age  tend  to  answer  correctly  for  the  first  three  pairs,  and  to 
make  the  same  answer  for  the  next  three.  That  is,  the  child 
points  for  the  first  three  pairs  to  the  longer  line  at  the  right; 
he  has  thus  established  a  "habit,"  and  follows  the  suggestion 
given  for  the  next  three  pairs  by  pointing  also  to  the  ones  at 
the  right. 

Binet  has  not  in  his  discussion  of  this  test  mentioned  the  fact 
that  frequently  the  wrong  judgment  on  the  part  of  the  child 
is  not  the  result  of  the  type  of  suggestibility  which  this  test 
is  designed  to  measure.  This  error  in  judgment  occurs  fre- 
quently under  such  circumstances  as  to  make  one  doubt  that  it 
is  the  result  of  habit  plus  suggestibility.  If  the  child  pauses 
for  a  moment  before  the  two  lines  of  equal  length,  looks  at 
and  scans  them  carefully,  and  then  indicates  one  as  being  longer 
than  the  other,  the  error  is  very  apparently  not  the  result  of 
"habit"  and  "suggestibility."  It  shows  that  he  has  used  his 
judgment  but  has  judged  incorrectly.  The  writer's  experience 
with  this  test  may  throw  light  upon  the  child's  error.  When 
looking  at  the  two  lines  intently  and  moving  the  eye  from  the 
left-hand  end  of  the  left  line  to  the  right-hand  end  of  the  right 


46  CLARA  SCHMITT 

line,  the  left  line  appears  to  be  the  longer.  Many  of  the  chil- 
dren to  whom  this  test  was  given  made  this  particular  type  of 
wrong  judgment.  The  number  is  indicated  in  the  tables  which 
follow.  Those  who  made  the  error  in  such  a  way  as  to  conform 
with  Binet's  interpretation  of  it,  that  is,  said  the  right  hand 
line  was  longer,  were  marked  failures  in  the  tables.  The  writer 
also  tried  this  test  upon  six  adults  who  came  into  the  clinic 
one  after  another  on  a  certain  day.  All  of  these  persons  made 
wrong  judgments,  at  least  to  the  extent  of  saying  that  they 
thought  the  left  line  was  longer  than  the  other  but  were  not 
quite  sure  about  it. 

II.  Cf.  above,  test  five,  under  ten-year-old  children. 

III.  Says  more  than  sixty  words  in  three  minutes. — The  child 
is  asked  to  say  as  many  words  as  he  can  think  of  in  three 
minutes,   and   is  told  that  they  will  be  counted.      Binet  says, 
"This  test  is  very  interesting,   for  its  fertility  in  suggestions. 
Besides  the  number  of  words,  one  can  know  their  relation.    Some 
subjects  give  only  detached  words,  each  of  which  requires  an 
effort  to  recall;  others  give  a  series  of  words,  the  furnishings 
of  a  school,  various  articles  of  clothing,  geological  terms,  etc. 
Some  use  only  names  of  common  objects,  others  cite  abstract 
words  or  rather  far-fetched  words.     All  this  gives  an  idea  of 
the  mentality  of  the  subject.     The  use  of  series  of  words,  and 
of  abstract  terms,   indicates   a  certain   amount   of   intelligence 
and  culture.  ...  By  this  test  we  are  able  to  estimate,  accord- 
ing to  observations  which  we  have  made  elsewhere,  both  the 
intellectual  activity  of  an  individual  and  his  verbal  type." 

In  addition  to  the  above  phases  concerned  in  the  judgment 
one  may  derive  from  this  test,  another  may  be  considered.  In 
the  writer's  opinion,  a  certain  paucity  of  words  in  the  perform- 
ance of  this  test  with  young  children  does  not  necessarily  indicate 
a  low  level  of  intellectual  activity;  indeed,  it  may  indicate  a 
high  level.  In  the  conversation  of  every-day  life  words  come 
not  singly  and  unattached,  but  are  the  result  of  associations 
which  the  purpose  of  the  conversation  brings  about.  Other 
associations  than  those  pertinent  to  the  subject  of  discussion 
are  inhibited  by  the  normal  person.  Without  a  purpose  for  the 


STANDARDIZATION  OF  TESTS  47 

use  of  words  they  may  not  occur  in  association.  Many  children 
like  to  comment  upon  their  successes  with  this  test  and  tell  how 
they  accomplish  it.  One  said,  "I  thought  about  all  the  things 
that  we  have  on  our  boat,  and  named  them  all."  Another 
fastened  his  eyes  on  a  map  on  the  opposite  wall,  and  mentioned 
all  of  his  observations  associated  with  it.  Many  of  them  make 
the  comment,  "It  was  hard  to  think  of  the  words."  All  of  this 
comment  on  the  part  of  the  child  shows  the  necessity  for  an 
object  and  a  use  for  the  word  before  associations  with  other 
words  can  be  made. 

IV.  Defines  abstract  terms. — The  terms  to  be  defined  are: 
chanty,  justice,  kindness.     See  III,  under  adults. 

V.  Derives  the  sense  of  a  sentence  the  words  of  which  are 
mixed. — A  card  is  given  to  the  child  with  the  words,  For — an — 
the — at — hour — early — we — country — started.    The  child  is  told 
that  here  are  the  words  of  a  sentence  which  were  mixed  up, 
and  that  if  he  puts  them  in  the  right  order  he  will  make  a 
good  sentence.    This  test  is  discussed  further  on  page  72. 

CHILDREN  OF  FIFTEEN  YEARS 

I.  Repeats  seven  digits. — This  test  is  made  in  the  same  manner 
as  that  which   requires  the   repetition  of   five   digits.      In  the 
writer's  experience  with  it  most  children  who  succeed  are  those 
who  after  the  first  or  second  failure  repeat  softly  to  themselves 
the  digits  as  they  are  given  by  the  experimenter.     This  provides 
the  child  with  an  added  memory  image  to  aid  in  recall. 

II.  Gives  three  rhymes. — The  child  is  asked  to  repeat  as  many 
words  as  he  can  think  of  that  rhyme  with  the  word  obey.    The 
writer's  experience  with  this  test  shows  that  the  success  attained 
with  it  depends  upon  the  word  which  is  chosen.     Younger  chil- 
dren will  readily  construct  a  rhyme  with  the  word  hill,  for  in- 
stance,   but   remain   mute    when   given   the   word    obey.     The 
two-syllable  and  more  unfamiliar  word  presents  to  their  minds 
difficulties  which  they  do  not  attempt  to  surmount.     The  same 
child,  however,  will  glibly  recite  hill,  fill,  will,  etc.,  when  given 
the  more  easy  and  familiar  word. 

III.  Repeats  a  sentence  of  twenty-six  syllables. — The  child  is 


48  CLARA  SCHMITT 

told  that  the  experimenter  will  repeat  some  sentences  to  him, 
and  that  he  is  then  to  repeat  them  exactly  as  he  has  heard 
them,  without  the  change  of  a  single  word.  The  writer's  ex- 
perience shows  that  success  with  such  sentences  is  dependent 
upon  the  familiarity  of  the  child  with  the  words  used.  An 
unfamiliar  word  or  name  so  attracts  the  child's  attention  ;from 
the  remainder  of  the  sentence  that  he  is  unable  to  give  it.  When 
giving  such  tests  to  children  of  foreign  parentage  it  has  been 
found  expedient  to  use  the  vernacular  to  which  the  child  is 
accustomed.  For  instance,  in  the  following  sentences :  The 
other  day  I  saw  on  the  street  a  pretty  yellow  dog;  Little  Morris 
has  stained  his  nice  new  apron.  Among  children  of  the 
street  type  frequently  encountered  Morris  is  sometimes  an  un- 
familiar name,  stained  is  always  an  unfamiliar  word.  The  test 
is  passed  better  by  these  children  if  these  words  are  changed 
in  such  a  way  as  to  make  the  sentences  seem  very  ifamiliar  to 
them.  In  the  following  sentence, — "Ernest  is  frequently  pun- 
ished for  his  bad  conduct" — frequently  is  a  word  which  the  street 
child  has  probably  never  used,  even  if  he  has  heard  it.  It  is 
expedient  to  change  it  to  a  word  familiar  to  him  in  his  own 
vocabulary. 

IV.  Interprets  a  picture. — See  above. 

V.  Solves  a  problem  from  several  facts. — The  two  situations 
presented  to  the  child  are :    ( i )  A  woman  walking  in  the  forest 
of  Fontainebleau  stopped  suddenly,  dreadfully  frightened,  hurried 
to  the  nearest  policeman  and  told  him  that  she  had  just  seen 
hanging  to  a  limb  of  a  tree — what?    (2)  My  neighbor  has  just 
received  some  singular  visitors.     He  received,   one  after  the 
other,  a  doctor,  a  lawyer  and  a  priest.     What  is  going  on  at  my 
neighbor's  house?     These  situations  are  presented  to  the  child 
in  such  a  way  as  to  conform  with  circumstances  familiar  to 
him.    The  name  of  the  park  nearest  his  home  is  substituted  for 
the   forest   of  Fontainebleau.      In   the   second   situation,    with 
Protestant  children  minister  is  substituted  for  priest,  and  with 
Jewish  children  Rabbi  is  substituted.    The  writer  has  considered 
it  expedient  to  allow  credit  for  answers  to  the  first  in  addition 
to  the  one  which  Binet  permits.     Binet  judges  the  only  correct 


STANDARDIZATION  OF  TESTS  49 

response  to  be,  "A  person  who  has  been  hanged."  The  answer 
sometimes  is  made.  In  addition,  one  often  obtains  the  answer, 
"A  wild  animal."  Under  the  circumstances,  this  is  in  the  child's 
mind  sufficient  reason  for  the  conditions  of  the  problem.  In  the 
city  of  Chicago  one  of  the  large  parks  contains  a  menagerie. 
There  occur  occasionally  in  the  newspapers  stories  of  the  escape 
of  animals  from  the  menagerie.  That  these  stories  are  always 
untrue,  of  course  the  child  does  not  know.  Another  answer 
which  has  been  given  so  many  times  as  to  show  the  application 
of  real  experiences  to  this  situation  is,  "An  owl;  his  eyes  frighten 
you  and  make  you  think  something  dreadful  is  there"  For  the 
second  situation,  the  writer  has  found  it  expedient  to  add  an 
additional  caution.  If  the  child  answers  correctly,  "He  is  ill," 
or  "He  is  dying,"  he  is  asked  to  tell  why  in  that  case  the  three 
people  have  gone  there.  Frequently  the  child's  correct  judgment 
is  made  with  reference  to  one  of  the  conditions  only,  that  the 
doctor  has  gone  there.  He  does  not  know  why  the  lawyer  and 
the  priest  have  gone,  which  shows  that  he  has  not  taken  into 
account  the  whole  situation.  His  answer,  even  if  correct,  is 
given  such  credit  only  if  he  can  give  correctly  the  functions 
of  the  lawyer  and  priest  in  the  situation. 

ADULTS 

I.  Solves  the  paper  cutting  test. — A  sheet  of  paper  is  folded 
along  both  diameters  before  the  subject  and  a  small  triangle 
is  cut  out  along  the  edge  which  shows  but  a  single  fold.     The 
subject  is  asked  to  draw  on  a  similar  sheet  before  him  the 
position  of  the  cut  out  portion  when  the  sheet  is  unfolded.    This 
test  requires  control  of  the  mental  imagery  in  accordance  with 
the  given  conditions,  such  as  to  bring  about  in  imagination  the 
correct  result  of  the  conditions. 

II.  Reconstructs  a  triangle. — A  card  is  cut  in  two  pieces  along 
the  diagonal.     The  pieces  are  placed  before  the  subject  on  a 
sheet  of  paper,  and  he  is  asked  to  draw  the  resulting  shape 
if  the  lower  piece  is  placed  in  such  a  way  that  the  short  side 
lies  along  the  diagonal  of  the  other  card  with  the  right  angle 
at  the  left-hand  corner  of  the  upper  card  and  the  end  of  the 


5o  CLARA  SCHMITT 

long  side  pointing  downward.  One  says  to  the  subject,  "How 
will  it  look  if  I  place  the  lower  card  so  that  this  edge  lies 
along  this  edge,  with  this  corner  here,  and  this  one  pointing 
downward?",  with  the  gestures  appropriate  to  the  above  ex- 
planation. This  test,  as  the  one  above,  is  one  of  control  of  the 
imagery  to  correspond  with  the  given  conditions,  with  a  con- 
crete stimulus  to  set  up  the  train  of  imagery. 

III.  Gives   difference   in   meaning    of   abstract    terms. — The 
question  is  asked,  What  is  the  difference  between  laziness  and 
idleness;  between  event  and  advent;  between  evolution  and  rev- 
olution?"   The  passing  of  this  test  depends,  of  course,  upon  the 
training  which  the  subject  has  received.    Except  among  educated 
classes  in  America  the  word  advent  is  unusual,  as  is  also  the 
word  evolution.     An  example  which  illustrates  the  dependence 
of  this  test  upon  training  is  the  answer  of  a  twelve-year-old 
Catholic  boy  in  the  high  school.     He  said,  "Advent  is  a  church 
festival;  evolution  is  a  term  in  arithmetic."     Both  these  answers 
were  correct,  though  strictly  they  could  not  fill  the  conditions 
of  the  test.     Many  children  say  for  revolution,  "It  is  a  turning 
about,"   often  giving  as   an  example,    "A  wheel   revolves   and 
then  there  is  a  revolution."    This,  to  be  sure,  is  correct.     Many 
children  give  as  a  definition   for  revolution,  "It  was  a  war," 
which,  with  reference  to  American  history,  is  also  correct. 

That  there  is  a  difference  in  innate  ability  between  the  per- 
sons whose  acquaintance  with  the  words  has  been  somewhat 
limited  and  who,  therefore,  gives  a  limited  definition  but  en- 
tirely correct  within  the  realms  of  his  own  experience,  and  the 
older  or  better  educated  person  who  gives  a  definition  for  the 
words  also  correct  within  the  realms  of  his  larger  experience, 
is  problematical.  If  the  two  are  of  the  same  age  but  the  educa- 
tion of  the  former  was  cut  off  at  such  a  place  that  further 
experience  with  these  words  was  prevented,  one  may  not  rate  him 
upon  this  test  with  less  innate  ability  but  with  less  education. 

IV.  Solves  the  question  concerning  the  president. — The  ques- 
tion is,  There  are  three  principal  differences  betzveen  a  king  and 
a  president;  what  are  they?     This  test  also  depends   for  its 
proper  performance  upon  the  education  of  the  subject. 


STANDARDIZATION  OF  TESTS  51 

V.  Summarizes  an  observation  made  by  Hervieu. — The  child 
is  told  that  a  short  paragraph  will  be  read  to  him,  and  then 
he  is  to  tell  in  his  own  words  the  meaning  of  it.  The  paragraph 
is :  Many  opinions  have  been  given  on  the  value  of  life.  Some 
call  it  good,  others  call  it  bad.  It  would  be  more  just  to  say 
that  it  is  mediocre;  for  on  the  one  hand  our  happiness  is  never 
so  great  as  we  would  have  it,  and  on  the  other  hand  our  misfor- 
tunes are  never  so  great  as  others  would  have  them.  It  is  this 
mediocrity  of  life  which  makes  it  just,  or  rather  which  prevents 
it  from  being  radically  unjust.  This  tests  the  ability  to  control 
the  attention  over  the  period  of  the  reading  of  the  selection 
and  to  generalize  the  abstract  thought  which  it  contains. 


IV 

FALLACIES  AND  INADEQUACIES  OF  THE 
BINET-SIMON   SERIES 

From  the  use  and  analysis  of  the  Binet-Simon  tests  one  realizes 
that  the  theory  underlying  their  construction  was  not  clearly 
conceived  by  their  authors  or  was  not  consistently  carried  out. 

The  method  used  in  the  first  series  was  that  of  putting  to 
children  of  different  ages  a  large  number  of  questions  and 
setting  down  as  suitable  to  each  age  those  questions  which  re- 
ceived at  a  given  age  a  certain  percentage  of  correct  answers. 
The  1908  series,  which  has  received  the  largest  use,  contained 
a  reading  test.  In  the  1911  series  this  reading  test  with  a  few 
others  of  less  importance  were  eliminated  in  order  that  the 
series  might  be  free  from  those  tests  which  are  the  product  of 
educational  advantage.  Because  of  these  considerations,  then, 
the  series  may  be  accepted  as  designed  to  measure  intellectual 
growth  from  year  to  year  without  reference  to  the  changes 
produced  by  formal  instruction. 

Though  Binet  nowhere  definitely  outlines  his  theory  one  gains 
the  impression  that  the  different  age  groups  of  tests  are  designed 
to  measure  something  in  mental  development  which  is  qualita- 
tively different  from  year  to  year.  One  infers  from  various 
statements  that  certain  tests  are  possible  at  nine  years  of  age, 
for  instance,  which  were  not  at  eight  because  of  a  certain  quality 
of  the  nine  year  mental  age  not  possessed  by  the  eight  year 
mental  age.  In  other  words  the  assumption  is  that  there  is  a 
mental  growth  from  year  to  year  which  makes  it  possible  to 
take  on  at  corresponding  ages  certain  experiences  without  ref- 
erence to  previous  experience.  For  instance,  at  a  certain  age 
it  is  possible  for  the  child  to  know  the  months  of  the  year, 
at  another  age  he  cannot.  That  is,  this  underlying  factor  of 
mental  growth  determines  the  form  of  expression  of  mental 
life.  The  converse  of  Binet's  theory  is  that  the  form  of  ex- 


STANDARDIZATION  OF  TESTS  53 

pression  of  mental  life  at  any  time  is  determined  by  the  sum 
of  previous  experiences. 

The  theory  of  Binet  may  be  expressed  as  follows:  at  a  cer- 
tain chronological  age  the  mental  age  may  be  represented  by 
x,  at  a  succeeding  chronological  age  it  is  y,  and  at  a  third 
it  is  z.  According  to  the  converse  theory,  the  mental  age  of  the 
first  period,  is  x,  at  the  second  x  -f-  i,  at  the  third  x  +  2. 
A  third  possibility  presents  itself.  At  the  earliest  measurable 
stage  of  development  the  mental  age  is  x,  at  the  second  it  is  y, 
and  at  a  third  it  is  z,  and  thereafter  at  succeeding  stages  it  is 
z  +  i,  £  +  2,  etc.  The  unknown  quantities  stand  for  the  im- 
measurable innate  factors  which  distinguish  the  vegetative  idiot 
from  the  normal  person  and  the  ordinates  for  the  measurable 
factors  of  experience. 

Apparently  Binet  assumed  the  first  possibility  to  be  the  rule 
of  development  from  infancy  to  adult  age.  That  the  assumption 
is  true  to  this  extent  has  not  been  proved  by  observation  or 
experimentation  in  child  psychology. 

It  is  probable  that  the  third  possibility  more  nearly  expresses 
the  truth.  There  are,  we  know,  periods  of  development  in  the 
child  where  great  and  significant  changes  take  place,  both 
physically  and  mentally.  The  acquisition  of  walking  and  talk- 
ing mark  stages  of  development  which  are  of  great  significance 
in  the  growing  child.  Certainly  the  mind  is  qualitatively  dif- 
ferent after  the  advent  of  the  great  increase  of  motor  ability 
accompanying  walking,  and  of  language.  The  advent  of  puberty 
marks  another  such  stage  of  development.  The  mental  changes 
accompanying  puberty  mark  off  a  rich  field  for  investigation. 
The  walking  stage  and  the  pubertal  stage  have  their  bases  in 
physiological  changes  which  may  be  more  or  less  definitely  ac- 
companied by  intellectual  changes.  To  what  extent  these  physio- 
logical changes  cause  or  accompany  or  are  paralled  by  intellectual 
changes  of  a  qualitative  kind  not  dependent  upon  previous 
experience,  is  one  of  the  unanswered  problems  of  genetic 
psychology.  Aside  from  these  few  possibilities  we  do  not  know 
whether  mental  development  proceeds  from  year  to  year  as 
Binet  assumed.  We  do  not  yet  know  except  in  a  few  matters 


54  CLARA  SCHMITT 

whether  a  child  is  innately  more  capable  of  certain  mental  pro- 
cesses at  one  time  than  at  another.  In  the  discussion  of  the 
preceding  pages  it  has  been  shown  that  some  of  the  tests  placed 
at  certain  ages  by  Binet  and  supposed  to  measure  abilities  peculiar 
to  those  ages,  could  be  used  to  call  out  an  expression  of  the 
same  abilities  at  earlier  ages  if  so  presented  as  to  fall  within 
the  child's  range  of  possible  experience  at  those  ages.  The 
interpretation  of  pictures,  counting  backward,  and  originating 
of  a  sentence  with  three  given  words  are  cases  in  point. 

Until  we  know  more  of  these  most  fundamental  of  the  under- 
lying facts  of  genetic  psychology  we  can  not  unqualifiedly  accept 
the  Binet-Simon  tests  for  the  purpose  for  which  they  were  de- 
vised, namely  the  measurement  of  mental  age.  We  must  know 
in  more  fundamental  terms  than  they  express  what  it  means 
to  be  eight  years  of  age,  or  ten  years  of  age  mentally. 

We  must  be  able,  too,  to  separate  innate  mental  development 
from  that  due  to  education  of  specific  types.  This  the  Binet- 
Simon  tests  fail  largely  to  do.  This  point  has  been  indicated 
in  the  foregoing  discussion  of  the  individual  tests.  The  most 
striking  example  of  this  lack  is  the  test  which  requires  the 
reciting  of  the  months  of  the  year.  The  very  young  or  the 
defective  individual  may  have  the  ability  to  learn  this  series  with 
more  or  less  facility  but  the  conception  of  time  relationship  in- 
volved in  the  series  is  one  which  it  is  possible  is  not  entertained 
by  either.  The  same  thing  may  be  said  of  the  counting  series 
which  may  be  learned  as  a  verbal  series  without  the  accompany- 
ing conception  of  number.  Such  tests  as  these  without  further 
investigation  fail  to  indicate  the  type  of  mental  complex  involved 
in  passing  them. 

It  was  Binet's  attempt  to  measure  only  innate  ability  as  dis- 
tinguished from  information,  however,  which  led  him  to  discard 
reading  tests  from  the  1911  series.*  By  reason  of  this  the 
series  now  fails  to  take  account  of  a  most  important  set  of 
abilities,  those  that  the  school  endeavors  to  develop.  Many 
innate  abilities  can  be  measured  only  by  the  reaction  of  the 
individual  to  the  learning  situation.  The  most  obvious  measure 
of  the  ability  or  group  of  abilities  which  enables  one  to  learn 


STANDARDIZATION  OF  TESTS  55 

to  read  is  the  amount  that  has  been  learned  after  a  given  period 
of  instruction, — say  a  year  in  the  first  grade  of  the  public  school. 
The  use  which  can  be  made  of  the  school  tests  of  reading, 
writing,  and  arithmetic  is  discussed  below.  They  are  valuable 
because,  in  a  given  situation  in  which  the  curriculum  and  the 
child's  history  are  known,  the  relation  of  the  product  to  the 
experience  can  be  measured. 

However,  if  we  should  admit  tfrat  there  is  such  development 
of  mental  age  as  may  be  measured  from  year  to  year,  the  Binet- 
Simon  tests  do  not  constitute  an  accurate  measure  of  it,  as  is 
claimed  by  Binet  and  his  followers.  This  is  most  strikingly  shown 
in  the  work  of  Goddard  (13)  who  applied  the  scale  of  1908 
to  two  thousand  nonselected  children  of  the  public  school.  He 
made  a  distribution  table  showing  the  relation  between  the  mental 
age  as  measured  by  the  Binet-Simon  tests,  and  the  chronological 
ages  of  this  group.  This  piece  of  work  was  discussed  in  1912 
(18)  by  the  writer  as  follows,  beginning  with  Goddard's  table 
showing  the  chronological  and  mental  age  distribution  of  his 
two  thousand  subjects. 


Ag 
4 
S 
6 

e 
yrs  
yrs  

yrs  

I        2        2        3 

2      4      8    40     16      4 
i       o      3    29    48    69 

9 

0 

i 

8 
114 
160 

7 

yrs  

o      i      2      8    15  114 

50 

4 

3 

197 

8 

yrs  

0        O        2        2        I      87 

86 

16 

12 

3 

209 

0 

yrs  

27 

54 

56 

58 

4 

2 

2OI 

10 

yrs  

3     IS 

24 

19 

124 

27 

8 

2 

222 

II 

prs  

14 

13 

25 

50 

00 

12 

I 

166 

12 

yrs  

4 

10 

13 

42 

,36 

39 

144 

13 

yrs  

li 

5 

6 

30 

IQ 

21 

7 

89 

14 

yrs  

i 

I 

6 

5 

4 

3 

2O 

15 

yrs  

3 

0 

ii 

2 

o 

6 

Showing  the  chronological  and  mental  age  distribution  of  the  two  thousand 
public  school  children  graded  by  the  Binet  tests. 

The   writer  has   computed   from  this   distribution   table   the 
percentages  of  those  who  passed  "at  age"  or  normal,  "below 

*Nouvelles  Researches  sur  la  Mesure  Niveau  Intellectual  chez  les  Enfants 
<TEcole.    L'A.  P.     17:  146. 


56  CLARA  SCHMITT 

age,"  and  "above  age"  for  the  different  ages.    These  percentages 
arrange  themselves  as  follows: 

Age                                                         Below  Age  At  Age          Above  Age 

5 12.2%  35-0%  52.6% 

6. 20.6  30.0  49.4 

7 13-2  57-8  28.9 

8 44.0  41.1  14.8 

9 40.2  27.8  31.8 

10 27.4  55.8  16.6 

ii 554  36.2                    7.8 

12 72.9  27.O  OO.O 

13 92.1  7.8  oo.o 

This  table  shows  that  with  the  exception  of  the  seven-year- 
and  the  ten-year-old  children  less  than  fifty  per  cent  of  any 
group  were  graded  "at  age"  according  to  the  Binet  scale.  Of 
the  eight,  nine,  eleven  and  twelve-year-old  children  the  largest 
group  is  of  the  "below  age"  group;  and  of  the  five-  and  six-year 
old  children  the  largest  is  the  "above  age"  group.  Dr.  Goddard 
has  grouped  all  the  "above  age,"  all  the  "below  age,"  and  the 
normal  or  "at  age"  groups  regardless  of  chronological  age  and 
obtains  a  curve  very  closely  approximating  a  normal  distribu- 
tion. Of  this  curve  Dr.  Goddard  says,  "The  significance  of  these 
figures  obtained  from  the  general  result  is  very  great.  There 
is  every  reason  to  believe,  and  statisticians  confirm  this,  that 
any  group  of  two  thousand  children  may  be  taken  as  a  fair 
sample  of  conditions  to  be  found  in  any  number  of  children 
to  be  found  in  any  country.  Consequently  whatever  percentages 
or  proportions  are  found  here  may  be  taken  to  be  very  closely 
the  standard  to  be  found  elsewhere."  In  answer  to  this  state- 
ment we  may  make  the  very  obvious  objection  that  this  curve  is 
not  made  up  of  the  measurement  of  one  quality  of  an  otherwise 
homogeneous  group,  but  is  compiled  from  the  measure  of  many 
qualities  of  children  of  different  ages.  It  is  made  up  of  the 
results  of  tests  applied  to  children  of  different  ages  who  may 
not  have  done  the  same  tests,  as  will  be  presently  shown.  The 
curve  can,  therefore,  have  no  statistical  validity.  It  is  merely 
a  happy  or  an  unhappy  accident.  Dr.  Goddard  says  further, 
"Bearing  this  in  mind  it  becomes  very  significant  when  we  find 
that  we  have  78  per  cent  of  the  children  practically  normal  and 
satisfactory — for  we  allow  those  children  who  are  one  year  above 


STANDARDIZATION  OF  TESTS  57 

and  one  year  below  to  pass  with  the  central  group  as  satisfactory 
children."  It  is  only  by  lumping  the  percentages  again  that 
this  approximation  of  a  normal  distribution  is  obtained.  The 
percentages  of  those  graded  normal  according  to  Dr.  Goddard's 
standard  for  the  different  ages  arrange  themselves  as  follows: 

Age 5         6         7         8         9        10       1 1         12       13  years 

Per  cent  normal 77.1     91.2    80.7    90.4    83.5     76.5     73.4    52.20    31.4 

This  table  shows  that  it  is  only  for  the  ages  five,  ten,  and 
eleven  that  a  standard  approximating  that  fixed  by  Dr.  Goddard 
is  obtained.  The  twelve  and  thirteen  year  group  fall  below, 
the  six,  seven,  eight,  and  nine  year  groups  above  it.  The  adop- 
tion of  Dr.  Goddard's  standard  is,  however,  hardly  permissible 
because  of  the  great  pliability  of  the  rule  for  grading  the  tests 
laid  down  by  Binet.  This  rule,  presumably  followed  by  Dr. 
Goddard,  allows  a  child  to  be  graded  normal  or  at  age  if  he 
misses  not  more  than  one  of  the  tests  designed  for  his  age.  If 
he  misses  two  of  the  tests  for  his  age  he  may  be  allowed  to 
substitute  tests  of  a  higher  age  and  still  be  graded  normal. 
This  method  gives  the  nine-year-old  children,  for  instance, 
opportunity  to  fail  on  any  two  of  the  six  tests  for  that  age 
and  substitute  any  three  of  the  sixteen  remaining  tests.  They 
are  then  graded  as  nine  years  of  age  mentally.  The  eight- 
year-old  children  may  pass  any  five  of  the  twenty-two  tests 
above  those  for  eight  years  and  are  then  graded  nine  years 
of  age.  The  ten-year-old  children  may  fail  to  pass  two  of 
the  ten-year-old  tests  and  not  a  sufficient  number  of  those  above 
to  compensate  and  are  then  graded  nine  years  old  mentally. 
In  this  way  we  may  obtain  one  mental  age  group  by  classing 
together  three  groups  who  have  done  different  things.  Dr. 
Goddard  gives  a  further  pliability  to  the  method  of  grading 
by  grouping  together  with  these  as  normally  satisfactory  two 
other  groups  who  have  done  still  other  things. 

Further  doubt  is  cast  upon  the  accuracy  of  the  tests  by  the 
fact  that  judgments  arrived  at  through  their  application  do  not 
coincide  with  that  of  the  school  concerning  the  same  subjects. 
Dr.  Goddard,  himself,  recognizes  this.  He  says,  "Analyzing 
our  data  so  as  to  show  where  each  individual  is,  we  find  that 


58  CLARA  SCHMITT 

the  case  is  not  as  favorable  as  we  suggested  in  the  previous 
paragraph,  that  many  children  who  are  normal  mentally 
[according  to  the  Binet  tests]  are  two  or  three  or  possibly 
four  years  behind  their  grade.  We  find  a  great  many  other 
children  who  are  mentally  dull,  not  as  far  behind  their  grade 
as  their  mentality  would  require.  We  find  still  worse  condi- 
tions among  those  who  are  ahead  of  their  age  mentally.  They 
are  not  correspondingly  ahead  of  their  grade.  In  other  words 
the  two  systems  do  not  agree  at  all.  Now  having  satisfied  our- 
selves that  the  Binet  scale  is  the  most  accurate  method  that  we 
have  of  determining  intellectual  ability  in  children,  the  question 
at  once  arises,  how  much  injustice  is  being  done  these  children 
by  the  ordinary  school  routine?"  The  teachers  of  the  school 
might  well  retort  to  this  question  that  as  they  have  the  child 
continuously  over  a  period  oif  years  their  judgment  of  Ms 
abilities  ought  necessarily,  in  general,  to  be  more  accurate  than 
that  arrived  at  by  a  ten  or  twenty-minute  examination  over 
very  little  of  the  matter  with  which  the  school  concerns  itself. 

Terman  and  Childs  (15),  after  the  application  of  the  Binet- 
Simon  scale  of  396  non-selected  children  of  the  public  school, 
came  to  the  following  conclusion :  "It  is  evident  from  the  results 
of  our  investigation  that  the  Binet  scale  requires  a  radical  revision 
to  make  it  at  all  suitable  to  conditions  in  this  country."     The 
revision  of  the  Binet  tests  made  by  Terman  and  Childs,  as  they 
point  out,  "has  made  the  lower  end  of  the  Scale  more  difficult 
by  setting  back  many  of  the  tests  of  Binet's  higher  years,  and 
the  upper  range  has  'been  supplemented  .  .  .  and  some  of  the 
tests  even  discarded  .  .  .  Believing  that  tests  of  memory,  vo- 
cabulary,   observation,    reasoning,    and    reaction   to   a   complex 
social  or  moral  situation  bring  out  fundamental  characteristics 
of  mental  ability,  we  have  given  our  scattered  range  of  tests 
on  memory,   questions   of   comprehension,    reasoning  tests   in- 
volving observation,  linguistic  invention,  and  association,  such 
as  the  completion  test,  and  rearranging  a  sentence  of  mixed 
words,  vocabulary,  etc.,  so  that  a  child  of  any  age  will  be  tested 
on  a  number  of  these  important  questions." 

Daugherty  (19)  applied  the  1911  series  to  483  public  school 


STANDARDIZATION  OF  TESTS  59 

children  with  the  following  results :  30  per  cent  passed  at  age, 
1 8  per  cent  above  age,  42  per  cent  below  age.  Of  the  483 
children,  45  per  cent  were  in  the  school  grade  normal  to  their 
chronological  age,  49.3  per  cent  were  retarded,  and  5.6  per  cent 
were  advanced.  When  distributed  according  to  mental  age  48.7 
per  cent  were  in  the  school  grade  normal  to  that  age,  21.1  per 
cent  were  retarded,  and  30.2  per  cent  were  advanced. 

Goddard  graded  four  hundred  institution  feeble-minded  with 
the  1908  series.  He  read  the  classified  grading  to  the  teachers 
and  other  officers  of  the  institution  and  asked  for  criticisms 
upon  the  classification.  The  object  of  the  exercise  was  to 
determine  whether  the  individuals  classed  together  by  the  Binet- 
Simon  system  would  be  so  classed  by  the  people  who  had  had 
school  and  other  types  of  acquaintance  with  them.  The  result 
was  that  no  individual  was  considered  by  the  majority  as  not 
belonging  to  the  class  in  which  his  name  was  presented  to  them. 
One  necessarily  must  doubt  the  validity  of  a  judgment  obtained 
under  such  circumstances  as  this.  The  classified  list  was  read 
to  the  teachers.  The  members  of  this  institution  doubtless  were 
already  prejudiced  in  favor  of  the  system  adopted  for  use  there, 
and  in  the  judgment  of  their  psychologist.  Their  minds  were 
not  left  free  for  unprejudiced  judgment. 

Kuhlman  (14)  asked  the  teachers  and  other  officials  to  grade 
fifty  institution  feeble-minded  into  five  groups.  These  children 
graded  from  eight  to  twelve  years  mental  age  by  the  tests.  Of 
the  result  he  says,  "The  most  striking  fact  about  this  table  is 
the  frequent  wide  range  of  disagreement  of  the  teachers'  grad- 
ings.  For  nine  children  these  grades  differ  by  four  years,  for 
nine  others  they  differ  by  three  years,  for  nineteen  by  two  years, 
and  for  seven  there  is  complete  agreement.  There  can  be  no 
question  about  the  fact  that  the  Binet-Simon  tests  do  not  make 
half  as  frequent  or  as  great  errors  in  the  mental  ages  as  are 
included  in  these  gradings  based  on  careful,  prolonged  obser- 
vation by  experienced  observers  on  this  class  of  children.  In 
other  words,  the  chances  for  error  with  the  tests  are  much 
less  and  are  smaller  when  they  do  occur  than  is  the  case  with  the 
grading  of  any  one  individual  experienced  observer  when  this 


60  CLARA  SCHMITT 

grading  is  on  the  usual  general  observation."  The  answer  to 
this  objection  is  that  there  are  many  considerations  to  enter 
into  the  estimation  of  the  intelligence  of  any  subject,  and  dif- 
ferent teachers  may  have  had  different  bases  for  their  standards 
of  judgment.  We  do  not  know  what  was  the  standard  of  the 
individuals  who  passed  judgment.  It  is  possible  that  each  teacher 
had  in  mind  that  subject  of  instruction  which  it  was  his  func- 
tion to  impart.  For  one  it  may  have  been  reading,  for  another, 
manual  training.  According  to  the  writer's  observation  ability 
of  defectives  in  the  two  subjects  varies  widely.  The  various 
Binet  series  provide  no  test  for  either, — if  one  takes  into  account 
the  rule  for  grading  for  the  series  prior  to  1911.  The  official 
concerned  with  the  institution  routine  work  'may  have  had  in 
mind  as  his  standard  the  reliability  of  the  child  in  such  work. 
The  value  to  be  placed  upon  any  judgment  of  general  mental 
ability  is  proportional  to  the  number  of  items  taken  into  con- 
sideration and  the  weighting  given  those  different  items.  We 
do  not  know  in  view  of  the  disagreement  whether  in  this  case 
it  was  the  judgment  of  some  of  the  teachers  or  the  rating 
arrived  at  by  the  use  of  the  Binet  tests  which  was  most  reliable. 
Since  there  was  so  great  disagreement  between  these  people 
who  were  well  equipped  by  experience  and  observation  to  make 
a  judgment  and  the  tests,  it  is  probable  that  the  former  took 
into  consideration  certain  factors  which  might  well  be  included 
in  any  system  of  mental  measurement. 

The  extensive  pieces  of  work  upon  the  Binet-Simon  tests 
quoted  above  show,  also,  the  lack  of  correlation  between  the 
series  and  the  child's  ability  to  succeed  with  the  work  of  the 
school.  The  Binet  tests,  therefore,  while  professing  to  test 
native  ability  are  concerned  very  little  with  the  education  which 
all  normal  children  have  the  native  ability  to  acquire,  and  which 
is  of  much  importance  in  civilized  life.  The  school  is  busy 
during  the  first  four  years  of  the  child's  school  life  developing 
ability  in  the  processes  of  reading,  writing  and  arithmetic.  In 
in  the  new  series  there  is  none  at  all.  The  arithmetic  tests  are : 
the  1908  series  there  was  no  reading  test  before  eight  years  and 
a  counting  test;  a  test  of  the  combination  of  the  numbers 


STANDARDIZATION  OF  TESTS  6 1 

2,  2,  2,  i,  i,  i ;  and  making  change  for  a  quarter.  The  school 
teaches  during  the  years  for  which  these  tests  are  designed 
more  complex  processes  than  these.  Along  with  and  after  the 
mastery  of  the  early  subjects  of  formal  instruction,  the  school 
is  concerned  with  their  application,  especially  of  reading,  to  the 
acquisition  of  a  systematized  body  of  information,  such  as  geog- 
raphy, history,  etc.  We  have  then  to  conclude,  that  since  the 
1908  series  falls  short  of  measuring  the  abilities  which  the 
school  expects  to  develop,  the  1911  series  is  still  more  open  to 
criticism. 

In  the  work  of  the  public  school  ability  to  read  is  of  the 
greatest  importance  because  upon  it  depends  all  further  progress 
in  the  school.  Number  conceptions  and  knowledge  of  the  pro- 
cesses of  their  combinations  are  of  so  great  importance  in  the 
practical  activities  of  every  day  life  that  arithmetic  occupies  a 
large  part  of  the  time  of  the  public  school.  Mentally  defective 
children  in  the  public  school  display  their  defectiveness  in  their 
slowness  or  failure  in  acquiring  the  processes  of  reading  and 
number  work.  Any  set  of  tests  which  fails  to  explore  these 
realms  of  mental  activity  can  be  of  little  value  as  a  measuring 
scale  for  backward  children  brought  to  the  clinic  of  the  public 
school.  We  must  conclude,  then,  that  at  least  the  lower  end 
of  the  Binet-Simon  scale  does  not  measure  the  ability  of  a 
child  in  accordance  with  the  social  standards  set  for  him. 

The  second  psychological  fallacy  implicit  in  the  grading  of 
mental  defectives  according  to  mental  age  is  seen  in  the  false 
assumption  that  a  defective  individual  of  any  age,  who  tests 
to  a  certain  mental  age  according  to  the  Binet-Simon  scale,  is 
equivalent  to  or  identical  with  the  normal  child  of  correspond- 
ing chronological  age.  Examples  which  illustrate  this  point  may 
be  quoted  from  the  clinical  studies  made  by  Huey  (20).  He  says 
of  case  22  :  "In  school  Hilda  reads  poorly  in  the  first  reader, 
adds  and  subtracts  very  little,  is  poor  in  spelling,  writing  and 
industrial  work,  but  dances  well.  She  gives  only  momentary 
attention  to  anything,  gets  on  fairly  well  with  others,  and  her 
worst  fault  is  stated  to  be  her  insistence  on  being  the  center  of 
attraction.  She  is  most  restless  and  'always  sits  on  one  leg  or 


62  CLARA  SCHMITT 

twisted  around  in  her  seat.'  She  appears  bright,  and  even 
spontaneous,  but  she  does  not  get  the  work  done.  She  is  over- 
demonstrative  of  her  affection  for  persons  whom  she  likes.  The 
Binet  tests  give  her  a  mental  age  of  eight  and  one-half  years,  a 
retardation  of  two  years.  She  could  not  repeat  16  syllables, 
could  not  count  stamps,  nor  backward  from  20  to  o,  could  not 
write  a  four  word  phrase  when  heard,  could  not  give  the  date 
even  approximately,  nor  make  change,  name  the  months,  or 
arrange  weights.  Hilda  has  learned  to  write  with  moderate 
legibility,  but  cannot  use  writing  to  any  purpose.  In  trying  to 
reproduce  stories  I  and  II  and  to  write  of  a  trip  in  a  flying- 
machine,  she  wrote  p,  6  and  4  lines  respectively,  being  a  hotch 
potch  such  as  'a  fat  pig  a  hoig  to  leand  a  good  heven  Cand  a 
sometime  cand!  etc.  Instead  of  writing  similars  and  opposites, 
in  the  tests  for  these,  she  either  copied  the  words  with  strange 
transpositions  and  changes,  or  occasionally  wrote  some  appar- 
ently unrelated  word  or  series  of  letters.  .  She  crossed  49  and  77 
A's  in  two  minutes  each,  with  no  errors.  Her  tapping  record 
counted  to  nearly  normal,  but  she  showed  exceedingly  poor  con- 
trol, tensing  her  fingers  into  knots,  hammering  the  key.  etc."  All 
of  this  description  points  out  in  a  very  striking  way  the  defects 
of  Hilda's  mentality  as  compared  with  that  of  the  normal  8- 
year-old  child  in  the  school.  The  normal  child  of  this  age  can 
do  more  than  read  poorly  in  the  first  reader,  has  a  knowledge 
of  arithmetic  processes  such  as  enables  him  to  make  changes 
within  one  dollar;  to  recognize  related  units  of  measure,  such 
as  inch,  foot;  minute,  hour,  day,  week;  pint,  quart;  cent,  nickel, 
dime,  quarter,  half-dollar?  dollar;  to  use  the  tables  of  two's 
and  three's;  to  count  by  two's  to  24  and  by  three's  to  36; 
to  tell  half  of  any  multiple  of  two  to  24  and  one-third  of  any 
multiple  of  three  to  36;  to  read  and  write  numbers  of  one  and 
two  orders ;  to  read  time  by  the  clock  to  hour,  half  hour,  quarter 
hour;  and  to  answer  any  of  the  45  addition  and  subtraction 
facts.  [According  to  the  1912  Course  of  Study  for  the  Second 
Grade  of  the  Elementary  Public  Schools  of  Chicago.]  The 
child  of  8  years  can  use  writing  to  some  purpose;  he  can  or- 
ganize his  mental  life  with  reference  to  this  accomplishment 


STANDARDIZATION  OF  TESTS  63 

so  that  he  does  not  produce  the  results  which  were  quoted  of 
Hilda. 

Another  example  of  this  fallacy  is  that  of  Robert  P.,  quoting 
from  the  volume  mentioned  above.  "In  school  Robert  reads 
fairly  in  the  first  reader  only,  does  some  addition  and  subtrac- 
tion, but  jailed  on  5  x  2  and  4x1.  He  does  well  in  calisthenics 
and  likes  to  'lead.'  He  is  also  good  at  dancing  and  in  basketry. 
In  manual  work  he  is  generally  quite  satisfactory,  only  working 
by  fits  and  starts,  though  occasionally  he  turns  in  and  works  hard 
for  a  time.  .  .  .  Mentally  Robert  shows  an  intelligence  of  nine 
years  with  a  retardation  of  five  and  one-half  years.  His  speech 
is  nasal,  but  he  can  articulate  normally.  -He  could  repeat  five 
numerals  but  once  in  seven  trials,  could  not  count  from  20  to  o, 
nor  make  change  of  4  cents  from  25,  name  the  months,  detect 
nonsense  in  sentences,  or  give  6  of  the  ip  details  about 
the  'fire.'  He  seems  to  be  bored  with  the  trouble  of  think- 
ing. He  did  not  make  absurd  replies,  but  was  merely  weak  in 
his  adaptations  and  at  the  same  time  rather  self-satisfied  with 
them.  'Not  very  hard3  was  his  characteristic  reply  after  utterly 
failing  to  rearrange  the  shuffled  words  of  a  sentence.  .  .  .  In 
the  written  tests  the  work  is  very  weak  both  in  quantity  and 
quality.  His  handwriting  is  irregular  almost  to  scribbling, 
though  large  and  therefore  moderately  legible.  His  mis-spell- 
ings, as  in  some  of  the  other  cases,  suggest  a  form  of  agraphia." 
It  is  needless  to  point  out  that  the  normal  child  of  nine  years 
can  do  more  than  read  in  the  first  reader,  would  not  fail  on 
5  x  2,  is  not  bored  with  the  trouble  of  thinking,  and  is  not 
satisfied  with  absurd  results,  such  as  were  mentioned  in  Robert's 
case. 

In  the  cases  described  above  one  also  sees  along  with  the 
fallacious  assumption  which  they  disclose,  the  failure  of  the 
Binet  tests,  alone,  to  adequately  describe  or  explore  the  mental 
life  of  any  subject.  This  inadequacy  of  the  Binet  tests  is  strik- 
ingly shown  in  the  description  of  a  case  which  was  discussed 
by  the  writer  in  the  above  mentioned  article: — "But  the  writer 
feels  impelled  to  assert  that  if  there  were  complete  agreement 
between  the  test  findings  and  school  grade  they  would  still,  alone, 
constitute  an  inadequate  measure  of  mental  ability  or  mental  de- 


64  CLARA  SCHMITT 

velopment.  The  best  possible  illustration  of  this  is  afforded  by 
the  description  of  a  boy  by  Holmes  in  a  recent  article.  The 
Classification  of  Clinic  Cases.  The  following  is  an  abstract  of 
Holme's  description  of  the  case :  The  case  was  that  of  a  six-year- 
old  boy  who  had  been  in  school  for  six-  months  without  having 
made  any  progress  in  the  work  of  the  school  in  spite  of  the  fact 
that  an  adult  sister  attempted  every  evening  to  teach  him  his 
lessons  for  the  next  day;  he  cried  when  struck  by  his  playmates 
or  when  hurt  by  his  playthings  but  did  not  strike  back  or  in  any 
way  try  to  defend  himself  and  would  run  to  his  mother  for  help; 
he  could  assemble  the  parts  of  electrical  apparatus,  arranging 
cells,  wires,  and  bells  so  they  would  ring;  could  connect  an  in- 
candescent lamp  so  it  could  be  lighted;  could  start  and  operate 
a  gas  engine  by  himself.  In  commenting  upon  this  case  Dr. 
Holmes  fell  in  the  fallacy  of  an  uncritical  acceptance  of  the 
Binet  tests  when  he  said,  'It  presaged  what  was  revealed  by 
the  Binet  tests,  namely  that  the  boy  was  one  year  beyond  the 
mental  attainment  of  the  average  boy  of  his  age,'  that  is,  he 
had  passed  the  Binet  tests  for  seven  years.  In  the  case  of 
this  boy  were  found  by  Dr.  Holmes  four  distinct  judgments. 
His  sister  and  the  school  thought  him  a  dullard ;  his  father,  with 
whom  he  worked  at  the  electrical  apparatus,  thought  him  all 
right;  his  playmates  considered  him  a  mollycoddle;  and  the 
Binet  tests  classified  him  as  somewhat  precocious.  No  two  of 
these  judgments  were  the  result  of  the  same  set  of  data.  The 
school  judged  him  by  his  proficiency  in  acquiring  the  processes 
of  reading,  writing,  and  number  conceptions;  the  Binet  tests 
have  nothing  to  do  with  these  school  abilities  except  counting 
to  thirteen  and  writing  from  copy  in  the  seven-year-old  tests, 
in  either  of  which  he  may  have  failed  and  still  be  graded 
one  year  ahead  of  his  age.  Neither  is  there  in  the  Binet  tests 
anything  which  would  hint  at  or  indicate  his  ability  with 
mechanical  contrivances;  nor  that  his  social  reactions  would 
be  as  they  were.  Should  the  school  and  social  disabilities  be 
persisted  in  through  life  or  for  several  years  he  certainly  would 
not  escape  being  considered  a  defective.  It  is  also  clearly  in- 
dicative of  the  inadequacy  of  these  tests  that  Dr.  Holmes  could 
not  give  a  description  of  the  case  in  terms  of  their  result.  He 


STANDARDIZATION  OF  TESTS  65 

had  to  resort  to  other  facts  in  order  to  present  a  true  picture. 
We  certainly  can  not  agree  with  him  that  this  boy  had  the  men- 
tal attainments  of  a  normal  boy  of  seven  years.  In  that  case 
we  should  have  to  believe  that  the  majority  of  seven-year-old 
boys  possess  his  mechanical  efficiency  and  his  academic  ineffi- 
ciency, which  is  not  true.  Children  of  six  can  learn  to  read 
and  if  children  of  seven  can  assemble  the  parts  of  a  gas  engine 
and  run  it  without  adult  supervision  no  one  knows  it.  In  the 
case  of  this  boy  there  was  one  and  possibly  there  were  two 
of  his  social  relationships  in  which  he  failed  to  function  prop- 
erly, namely,  in  his  reaction  to  the  school  and  to  his  playmates. 
It  would  seem  to  be  the  legitimate  business  of  the  psychological 
clinic  to  find  why  he  thus  failed.  Was  the  defect  in  his  school 
work  due  to  a  lack  of  ability  for  that  type  of  activity,  to  lack 
of  interest  in  it;  or  was  it  a  result  of  his  defective  social  re- 
sponse? And  what  was  the  cause  of  this  latter  defect? 

Another  case  showing  the  inadequacy  of  the  Binet  tests  to 
describe  a  case  of  mental  defect  was  described  by  the  writer 
in  the  above  mentioned  article  as  follows :  "This  case  was  a 
boy,  Frank,  aged  sixteen.  Following  are  his  reactions  to  the 
Binet  series  copied  from  the  notes  of  the  writer  made  as  they 
were  given. 
Eight  Year  Tests : 

1.  Comparison  of  butterfly  and  fly,  etc.,  passed. 

2.  Counts  backwards,  passed  (?)   (Forgot  where  he  was  once 

and  had  to  ask  what  had  said  last). 

3.  Notes  omission  eyes,  etc.,  passed. 

4.  Date,  failed. 

5.  Repeats  five  numerals,  passed   (once  out  of  three  trials). 

Nine  Year  Tests : 

1.  Makes  change,  passed  (25  cents — 9  cents.    Instead  of  mak- 

ing the  change  told  that  one  could  receive  it  in  the  smallest 
number  of  pieces  in  a  nickel,  a  dime,  and  a  penny). 

2.  Definitions  superior  to  use,  failed. 

3.  Recognizes  money,  passed. 

4.  Months  of  the  year,  failed. 

5.  Problem  situations,  passed. 


66  CLARA  SCHMITT 

Ten  Year  Tests: 

1.  Arranges  weights,  passed. 

2.  Copies  design,  passed. 

3.  Detects  incongruities,  failed. 

4.  Problem  situations,   failed. 

5.  Three  given  words   in  a  sentence,    failed    (Chicago   has 

money  in  the  river}. 
Twelve  Year  Tests : 

1.  Resists  suggestion,  passed. 

2.  Three  words  in  a  sentence,   failed. 

3.  Utters   60    words   in  three   minutes,    failed;    (27   words. 

Pauses  much,  though  urged  to  go  fast). 

4.  Definitions,   failed.      (Charity?     "Don't  know."     Justice? 

"Justice  of  the  peace."    Goodness?    "Gracious.") 

5.  Rearranges  shuffled  words  in  a  sentence,  failed. 

According  to  the  Binet  series  this  boy  grades  nine  years  of 
age,  and  it  might  be  thought  is  a  fit  candidate  for  the  feeble- 
minded institution.  The  further  disabilities  which  these  tests 
do  not  disclose  are  as  follows: 

1.  He  cannot  recognize  any  printed  words  and  not  all  of 
the  alphabet,  though  kept  in  school  the  regulation  time. 

2.  He  can  write  only  his  own  and  his  brother's  names.     Told 
to  write  the  cat  ran  away  wrote  the  set,  though  he  could  spell 
cat  correctly. 

3.  He  can  do  simple  number  combinations  such  as  5  plus  6, 
10  minus  4,  by  counting  his  fingers. 

4.  He  knows  only  that  his  birthday  comes  in  the  summer; 
said,  "My  mother  told  me  but  I  always  forget." 

5.  He  has  very  poor  control  of  associations  which  do  not 
provide  a  sense  stimulus  as  is  shown  in  his  reactions  to  the 
opposite  test.     Out  of  20  stimulus  words  he  reacted  correctly 
to  only  6,  gave  a  wrong  association  for  10,  and  failed  entirely 
for  4  of  the  stimulus  words. 

6.  He  is  very  suggestible.     In  the  Aussage  test  accepted  5 
out  of  7  suggestions. 

The  positive  abilities  which  this  boy  possesses  and  which  the 
Binet  tests  cannot  disclose  are: 


STANDARDIZATION  OF  TESTS  67 

1.  He  can  do  a  test  involving  the  planning  of  a  complex  set 
of  spatial  relationships  in  a  planned  and  comprehensive  way. 
This  ability  is  disclosed  by  test  IV  of  the  Healy-Fernald  series. 

2.  He  has  the  ability  to  do  in  a  planned  and  comprehensive 
way  a  test  requiring  the  analysis  of  the  functional  relationships 
of  a  simple  mechanical  contrivance,  as  is  disclosed  by  test  V. 
of  the  Healy-Fernald  series.     He  also  did  tests  I,  II,  and  III 
of  this  series  in  the  manner  considered  intelligent  in  the  grading 
adopted  in  the  work  of  the  clinic. 

The  history  of  the  boy's  industrial  life  correlates  with  the 
inference  that  one  might  draw  from  his  performance  with  the 
mechanical  tests.  He  has  been  an  efficient  farm  laborer  for 
some  months,  worked  satisfactorily  for  a  creamery  for  a  time 
loading  and  unloading  cans  etc.,  and  as  a  janitor  for  a  small 
school  building.  He  is  capable  of  earning  his  living  without 
direct  supervision." 

The  faults  of  the  Binet-Simon  series  may  then  be  summed  up 
as  follows : 

1.  The  assumption  of  serial  mental  development  from  early 
childhood  to  adult  age. 

2.  The  omission  of  tests  of  socially  significant  abilities. 

3.  Failure  to  distinguish  certain  innate  abilities  from  a  cer- 
tain expression  of  them  due  to  age  or  experience. 

4.  Is   not  an  accurate    measure   of   mental    development    of 
normal  children. 

5.  The  assumption  that  a  defective  is  quantitatively  rather 
than   qualitatively   different   from  a   normal   individual.     This 
point  is  discussed  further  on  p.  164. 

There  is  a  further  lack  in  the  series  which  has  been  implied 
in  the  description  of  Frank.  With  the  Binet  series  alone  one 
might  have  had  no  hint  as  to  his  industrial  possibilities.  The 
mechanical  tests  of  the  Healy-Fernald  type,  it  is  possible,  may 
be  made  to  supply  such  deficiency.  To  determine  this,  studies 
for  the  purpose  of  correlating  them  with  the  handwork  of 
the  school  or  other  places  where  such  activities  can  be  measured 
need  to  be  made. 


V 
DISCUSSION  OF  BINET-SIMON  TABLES 

The  reactions  to  the  Binet-Simon  tests  have  been  summarized 
in  the  following  seven  tables.  Each  table  is  arranged  with  refer- 
ence to  grade,  one  for  each  grade.  The  first  column  of  num- 
bers at  the  extreme  left  of  each  table  refers  to  the  individual 
children.  The  age  of  each  child  is  in  the  second  left  hand 
column,  and  the  results  of  the  individual  tests  are  recorded  in 
the  following  columns.  A  plus  sign  indicates  success  and  V 
failure  with  the  tests  according  to  the  Binet-Simon  grading, 
and  where  modifications  from  the  French  are  required  those 
adopted  by  Goddard  are  followed.  Where  the  author  has 
further  modified  the  standard  for  grading  has  been  indicated  in 
the  text  of  discussion  and  in  the  case  of  some  tests  by  footnote 
to  the  tables. 

The  tests  were  given  with  the  Healy-Fernald  tests  to  the  group 
of  children  described  on  page  2.  These  children  were 
considered  normal  by  the  teachers  who  had  them  in  charge.  No 
child  known  to  be  defective  or  seriously  backward  is  admitted 
to  the  school.  There  were  some  retarded  members,  the  extent 
of  which  is  shown  in  table  VIII  below.  The  causes  assigned 
by  the  teachers  for  retarded  cases  were,  in  general,  illness,  de- 
layed start  to  school  because  of  the  theory  of  the  parent  that 
such  a  course  was  best  for  the  child,  and  the  interruption  of 
regular  study  by  travel. 

The  general  technique  of  procedure  was  adopted  with  reference 
to  demands  of  the  Healy-Fernald  tests.  It  is  discussed  further 
on  page  86. 

-  In  the  conduct  of  the  two  sets  of  tests  the  Binet-Simon  tests 
were  reserved  for  the  last.  By  the  time  they  were  reached  the 
child  had  been  doing  tests  for  an  hour  or  more.  In  some  cases 
there  was  too  much  restlessness  and  fatigue  to  carry  the  child  as 
far  as  the  majority  of  his  comrades  in  his  grade  were  able  to  go 


STANDARDIZATION  OF  TESTS  69 

and  the  tests  were  then  discontinued.     This  accounts  for  the 
unevenness  of  the  right  hand  side  of  some  of  the  tables. 

Binet-Slmon  Record  of  Kindergarten. — Table  I  shows  the 
record  made  by  the  twenty-four  kindergarten  children.  A 
glance  at  the  table  for  the  tests  below  nine  years  shows  that 
the  greatest  amount  of  failure  occurs  with  the  following  tests: 
the  6-3  test,*  six  failures  to  copy  the  lozenge.  When  watching 
these  children  work  at  this  test  one  comes  to  the  conclusion 
that  the  failure  is  due  to  lack  of  muscular  control.  The  fre- 
quently heroic  and  often  unsuccessful  attempt  to  draw  the 
slanting  lines  of  the  lozenge  is  easily  apparent.  Seven  fail  at 
7-3,  describe  a  picture;  15  at  7-4,  give  the  value  of  nine  cents; 
9  at  8-2,  to  count  backward. 

The  counting  backward  test  was  not  an  utter  failure  on  the 
part  of  any  child  graded  V.  Those  so  graded  were  able  to 
comprehend  the  problem  sufficiently  well  to  make  a  reasonable 
attempt  at  it,  and  to  get  more  than  two-thirds  of  the  required 
terms  correct.  The  errors  were  mostly  those  of  omission.  They 
come  about  in  this  way;  the  child  has  successfully  reached  per- 
haps fifteen  in  his  backward  progress  toward  one,  and  here 
he  pauses  to  go  through  the  process  discussed  above  by  which  he 
determines  the  next  term  in  the  series.  He  counts  up  from 
some  term  nearer  one  and  having  come  up  to  fifteen  again, 
says  thirteen  instead  of  fourteen. 

Twenty-four  children  fail  at  8-4,  the  date;  nine  at  8-5,  to 
repeat  five  digits.  The  7-4  and  8-4  tests  concern  themselves  with 
bits  of  specific  instruction  not  included  in  the  curriculum  of  the 
kindergarten. 

Of  the  nine  year  tests  the  first  four  are  tests  of  the  results  of 
specific  school  instruction.  The  9-2  test,  defines  in  terms  super- 
ior to  use,  may  'be  classed  as  such  because  of  the  usual  school 
exercise  of  defining  words  found  in  the  reading  and  other  exer- 
cises of  the  school.  The  fifth  one  is  a  test  which  involves  having 
formed  a  generalized  rule  of  action  for  a  given  situation.  Of 
the  eighteen  children  who  were  given  the  nine  year  tests,  failure 
was  the  rule  with  the  first  four.  Eleven  of  the  eighteen  were 
*This  convention  is  adopted  to  indicate  test  III  of  the  six  year  group. 


G  ^M     «U 

^2    g> 


VOOO    txt^OOOO\OOO    txOO 


bobobobobobobobo&otiobo 


0 


bobcbflbflbflbobflbflbobo         bobobo 


OO    O 
invo 


O    "-"    O\ 
\O    ^"n 


HI     \O      tX 

«n  co  TJ- 

—        h-        >- 


^-     V 

a  % 


ON   ON  00  00         00    ON  00  00 


O 


$ 

H 

I 

•V* 

<? 


B 


o 


\O  \O  VO  VO 


0606 


O\000000    Q 
c^   o\oo   o<   o\ 


•c 


M"^ 
S  w 


72  CLARA  SCHMITT 

able  to  so  generalize  experience  into  rules  of  action  as  to  succeed 
with  the  fifth  test  according  to  the  Binet  standard.  No  child  of 
the  eighteen  failed  to  answer  at  least  one  of  the  problems 
correctly. 

Binet-Simon  Record  of  the  First  Grade. — Passing  to  the  record 
of  the  first  grade,  Table  II,  we  find  that  here  again  the  tests  below 
ten  years  which  depend  upon  specific  instruction  are  usually  not 
passed  except  the  7-4  test.  The  material  of  this  test  is  included  in 
the  curriculum  of  this  grade.  The  9-5  test  of  experience  is  univer- 
sally passed.  Perhaps  one  may  venture  upon  the  theory  that  it 
is  because  the  children  of  the  first  grade  have  had  more  and 
broader  social  experience  than  those  of  the  kindergarten.  The 
10-3,  10-4,  and  10-5  tests,  are  passed  by  approximately  two  thirds 
of  the  first  grade  children.  There  is  almost  entire  failure  with  the 
10-2,  test  of  visual  memory,  and  more  than  fifty  per  cent  of 
failure  with  the  discrimination  of  weight. 

Binet-Simon  Record  of  the  Second  Grade. — With  the  second 
grade,  Table  III,  the  reaction  to  the  10-3,  10-4,  and  10-5  tests 
remains  the  same  as  for  the  first  grade,  as  do  practically  the 
weight  discrimination  and  visual  memory  tests.  The  12-4  test, 
definition  of  abstract  terms,  is  generally  missed,  and  the  12-5, 
test  of  rearrangement  of  words  to  make  a  sentence.  This  test 
was  graded  V  in  accordance  with  Binet's  standard  of  the  time 
factor,  failure  in  one  minute.  Many  of  the  children  were  given 
more  time  and  several  trials  and  finally  accomplished  the  result. 
It  is  characteristic  of  the  child  who  fails  according  to  the  Binet 
standard  that  he  is  unable  to  see  the  sentence  entirely.  He  puts 
a  few  words  together  in  the  right  relation  and  a  few  others  in 
their  right  relation,  and  then  he  finally  criticises  the  whole  pro- 
duct. In  no  case  was  a  child  who  failed  to  do  the  test  satisfied 
with  his  result. 

This  is  in  striking  contrast  with  the  behavior  of  the  defective 
child.  The  latter  does  not  criticize  his  failures.  If  he  tries  to 
perform  the  test  he  is  usually  satisfied  with  the  result. 


C  v 

B  *a  3 


o  o  o  o  o 


O 


o 
c 

m 

o 

:§s 

'•3  M 


«j  c 


O 


bo 


2    £  N    ^00    ON  2 


OO    tx 
coN 


00  00 


oo    co  m  Th  vp 
<N     c?4     (^     f»     fN 


74 


CLARA  SCHMITT 


Binet-Simon  Record  of  Third  Grade. — The  children  of  the 
third  grade,  Table  IV,  continue  in  large  numbers  to  fail  in  the 
10-1,  10-2,  12-4,  and  12-5  tests.  It  is  interesting  to  note  that  the 
reaction  to  the  12-1  test  of  suggestion  has  changed  in  character. 
The  second  grade  child  made  no  error  in  his  judgment,  but  the 
third  grade  child,  perhaps  in  his  desire  to  exercise  great  care,  fell 
into  the  error  of  judgment  which  has  been  discussed  above. 

TABLE  IV 

Reaction  of   Third   Grade  Children  to   Binet-Simon   Tests 
Binet  Ages 


8  yr. 

9  yr. 

10  yr. 

12  yr. 

Grade 

Men- 
tal 
Age 

No. 

Age 

12345 

12345 

12345 

12345 

20 

8 

+  +  +  +  + 

+  +  +  v  + 

+  +  +  +  + 

+  +  V  V  V 

3 

i&A 

21 

8-2 

+  +  +  +  + 

+  +  +  +  + 

+  +  +  +  + 

+  +  +  +  v 

3 

10^ 

6 

8-8 

+  +  +  +  + 

+  +  +  +  + 

V  V  +  +  + 

+  +  +  V  V 

3 

^A 

12 

8-9 

+  +  +  +  + 

+  +  +  +  + 

v  +  +  +  + 

+  +  +  +  v 

3 

103/s 

IO 

8-10 

+  +  +  +  + 

+  +  +  +  + 

+  +  +  +  + 

+  +  +  v  + 

3 

io$i 

2 

9 

+  +  +  +  + 

+  +  +  +  + 

v  v  +  +  + 

+  +  v  +  v 

3 

10^ 

3* 

9 

+  +  +  +  + 

+  +  +  +  v 

+  v  +  +  +t 

+  V  V  V  V 

3 

9^ 

13 

9-2 

+  +  +  +  + 

+  +  +  v  + 

+  v  +  +  + 

+  +  v  +  v 

3 

™y5 

5 

9-2 

+  +  +  +  + 

+  +  +  +  + 

+  +  +  +  + 

+  +  V  V  V 

3 

io2/5 

7 

9-2 

+  +  +  +  + 

v  +  +  +  + 

v  v  +  +  + 

+  +  +  +  v 

3 

ioy5 

8 

9-2 

+  +  +  +  + 

+  +  +  +  + 

+  +  +  +  + 

+  +  +  v  v 

3 

i&A 

i 

9-3 

+  +  +  +  + 

+  +  +  +  + 

v  v  +  +  + 

+  +  v  +  v 

3 

ioH 

ii 

9-4 

+  +  +  +  + 

+  +  +  +  + 

+  v  +  +  + 

+  +  +  +  v 

3 

103/5 

4 

9-4 

+  +  +  +  + 

+  +  +  +  + 

+  v  +  +  + 

+  +  +  v  v 

3 

l02/5 

9 

9-4 

+  +  +  +  + 

+  +  +  +  + 

v  +  +  +  + 

+  +  +  +  v 

3 

V03/5 

17 

9-9 

+  +  +  +  + 

+  +  +  +  + 

+  v  +  +  + 

+  +  +  v  v 

3 

™2/S 

16 

9-9 

+  +  +  +  + 

+  +  +  +  + 

v  +  +  +  + 

+  +  +  +  V 

3 

103/5 

18 

9-KI 

+  +  +  +  + 

+  +  +  v  + 

+  +  +  +  + 

+  +  +  +  v 

3 

10^ 

14 

IO-2 

+  +  +  +  + 

+  +  +  +  + 

v  +  +  +  + 

+  +  V  V  V 

3 

10}* 

15 

10-4 

+  v  +  +  + 

+  +  +  +  + 

+  +  +  +  + 

+  +  +  +  + 

3 

12-11 

19 

10-4 

+  +  +  +  + 

+  +  +  +  + 

v  +  +  +  + 

+  4-  V  V  V 

3 

10^ 

+  Wrong  judgment. 

t  V  with  Chicago,  money,  river,        with  boy,  river,  ball. 
*  Considered  a  pathologically  timid  case,  but  not  lacking  in  ability, 
apparently  due  to  fear  of  expressing  a  wrong  judgment. 


V's 


STANDARDIZATION  OF  TESTS 


75 


Binet-Simon  Record  of  Fourth  Grade. — The  fourth  grade, 
Table  V,  shows  much  the  same  type  of  reactions  as  the  third 
grade. 

TABLE  V 

Reaction  of  Fourth  Grade  Children  to  Binet-Simon  Tests 
Binet  Ages 


No. 


Age 


8  yr. 


12345 


12345 


10  yr. 


12  yr. 


12345       12345 


Grade 


Men- 
tal 
Age 


40 
47 
53 
56 
51 
48 

45 
50 
55 
52 
46 
49 
54 
57 
44 
61 

59 
60 

58 
63 

62f 


9 

9-5 
9-9 
9-9 
9-9 
o-io 
9-10 

10 
10- 1 
IO-2 
IO-2 

10-4 
10-4 
io-5 
10-5 
10-5 
10-8 
lo-g 
10-9 

1 1-2 

n-6 

12-2 


+ 


+  +  v  + 
v  +  +  + 


v  +  +  +  + 


V  +  ++ 


v  +  +  +  + 


+  v  +  +  + 

+  V+  +  + 

v  +  +  +  + 
v  +  +  +  + 
v  v  +  +  + 
v  +  +  +  + 


V  V  +  +  + 

-f  +  +  +  + 
+  v  +  +  + 
v  v  +  +  + 

V  V  +  +  + 
V  V  +  +  + 

+  v  +  +  + 
+  v  +  +  + 

+  V+  +  + 

+  v  +  +  + 
v  +  +  +  + 

V  +  4-  +  + 


v  +  +  v  + 
+  +  +  v  v 

V  +  +  +  V 

+  +  +  +  v 
+  +  +  +  v 

+  V  +  +  V 

+  +  +  v  v 

+  +  +  +  v 
v  +  +  +  v 

+  +  +  +  v 

+  +  +  4-  v 

+  +  +  v  v 
+  +  +  +  v 
+  +  +  +  v 


+  +  + v 
+  +  +  v 


10?* 


105* 
105* 

ioy5 


io3/s 
105* 
io?* 
10?* 

105* 


12 


+  Wrong  judgment,  opposite  to  suggestion  judgment. 

15  yr. 
fAdd  to  No.  62  one  column  as  follows:  V  +  V   V 


CLARA  SCHMITT 


Binet-Simon  Record  of  Fifth  Grade. — The  fifth  grade,  Table 
VI,  shows  itself  capable  of  doing  the  12-5  test  and  fails  in  large 
numbers  in  the  10-1  and  10-2  tests.  The  15-3,  test  of  memory 
of  sentence  of  twenty-six  syllables,  and  the  15-4,  interpretation  of 
a  picture,  were  generally  failure. 

TABLE  VI 

Reaction  of  Fifth  Grade  Children  to  Binet-Simon  Tests 
Binet  Ages 


9  yr. 

10  yr. 

12  yr. 

15  yr. 

Grade 

Mental 
age 

No. 

Age 

12345 

12345 

12345 

i    2    3    4    sf 

66 

10-3 

+  +  +  +  + 

v  +  +  +  + 

+  +  +  +  + 

+  +  +  V  B 

5 

12^/5  or  11% 

65 

10-3 

+  +  +  +  + 

+  +  +  +  + 

+  +  +  +  + 

+  +  +  V  + 

5 

1  2^5 

64 

10-5 

+  +  +  +  + 

+  v  +  +  + 

+  +  +  +  + 

+  +  V  V  V 

5 

12%          II 

67 

10-6 

+  +  +  +  + 

v  v  +  +  + 

+  +  +  +  + 

V  V  V  V  V 

5 

12              IO% 

69 

10-6 

+  +  +  +  + 

v  v  +  +  + 

+  +  +  +  + 

+  +  v  +  + 

5 

12^          11% 

72 

ii 

+  +  +  +  + 

v  +  +  +  + 

+  +  +  +  + 

+  +  V  V  B 

5 

12%          II 

71 

1-1-2 

+  +  +  +  + 

+  +  +  +  + 

+  +  +  +  v 

+  +  v  v  + 

5 

12%          11% 

73 

I>I  -2 

+  +  +  +  + 

+  v  +  +  + 

+  +  +  +  + 

V  +  V  V  + 

5 

12%          II 

68 

n-5 

+  +  +  V  + 

+  v  +  +  + 

+  +  +  +  v 

V  +  V  +  A 

5 

10^          10^ 

70 

n*5 

+  +  +  +  + 

+  v  +  +  + 

+  +  +  +  + 

+  +  V  V  + 

S 

12%          11% 

74 

11-6 

+  +  +  +  + 

v  +  +  +  + 

+  +  +  +  +• 

V  +  V  V  + 

5 

12%          II 

78 

1  1-6 

+  +  +  +  + 

+  v  +  +  + 

+  +  +  +  + 

+  +  V  V  A 

5 

12%          II 

79 

11-8 

+  +  +  +  + 

+  v  +  +  + 

+  +  +  +  + 

+  +  V  V  A 

5 

12%          II 

77 

12 

j_  +  +  +  + 

+  +  +  +  + 

+  +  +  +  v 

+  +  v  +  + 

5 

12%          11% 

76 

I2-I 

+  +  +  +  + 

+  v  +  +  + 

+  +  +  +  + 

+  +  v  v  + 

5 

12^          11% 

75 

12-2 

+  +  +  +  + 

v  v  +  +  + 

+  +  +  +  + 

V  +  V  V  -f- 

5 

12%          10% 

83 

12-6 

+  +  +  +  + 

+  v  +  +  + 

+  +  +  +  + 

V  V  V  V  B 

5 

12              IO% 

80 

12-7 

+  +  +  +  + 

v  v  +  +  + 

+  +  v  v  + 

+  V  V  V  + 

5 

iof<5       10% 

82 

12-7 

+  +  +  +  + 

+  +  +  +  + 

+  +  +  +  v 

V  +  V  V  -f 

5 

12%          11% 

84 

12-11 

+  +  +  +  + 

v  v  +  +  + 

5 

9% 

81 

12-11 

+  +  +  +  + 

+  v  +  +  + 

+  +  +  +  + 

v  +  v  v  + 

5 

12%          II 

85 

13-8 

+  +  +  +  + 

+  v  +  +  + 

+  +  +  +  + 

+  +  v  v  + 

5 

uM      «.« 

+  Wrong  judgment. 

t  A  and  B  indicate  problems  i  and  2,  respectively,  of  the  test  passed. 


STANDARDIZATION  OF  TESTS 


77 


Binet-Simon  Record  of  Sixth  Grade. — The  reactions  of  the 
sixth  grade,  Table  VII,  were  much  the  same  as  those  of  the  fifth. 
The  adult  tests  were  given  to  the  sixth  grade  as  a  class,  the 
results  being  written  by  the  children  and  handed  in.  Of  the 
adult  tests,  number  2,  rearranges  a  triangle,  and  3,  give  differ- 
ences in  meanings  of  abstract  terms,  were  answered  by  two  thirds 
of  the  class.  The  matter  of  test  four  is  not  a  part  of  the  organ- 
ized civics  work  of  this  grade,  and  the  information  in  sufficiently 
organized  form  to  permit  of  an  attempt  to  answer  the  question 
must  have  been  obtained  through  general  reading  by  those  who 
succeeded. 

TABLE  VII 

'Reaction  of  Sixth  Grade  Children  to  Binet-Simon  Tests 
Binet  Ages 


12  yr. 

15  yr. 

Adult 

Grade 

Mental 
age 

No. 

Age 

12345 

12345 

12345 

H5 

12 

+  +  +  +  + 

+  +  +  +  + 

+  +  +  +  + 

6 

A 

116 

12 

+  +  +  +  v 

+  +  +  +  + 

v  +  +  +  + 

6 

15^ 

124 

12 

+  +  +  +  V 

V  +  V  V  + 

+  V  V  +  + 

6 

12 

105 

I2-I 

+  +  +  +  + 

+  +  v  +  v 

v  +  +  v  + 

6 

12^ 

109 

I2-I 

+  +  +  +  + 

+  +  +  +  + 

+  +  +  v  v 

6 

l53/5 

104 

I2-I 

+  +  +  +  + 

+  +  v  +  + 

+  v  +  +  + 

6 

153/S 

123 

I2-I 

+  +  +  +  + 

+  +  v  v  + 

v  +  +  v  + 

6 

I5H 

in 

12-2 

+  +  +  +  + 

+  V  V  V  + 

V  +  V  V  + 

6 

12^ 

IOO 

12-2 

+  +  +  +  + 

+  +  v  +  v 

+  +  +  +  v 

6 

l52/5 

114 

12-3 

+  +  +  +  + 

+  +  v  +  + 

+  +  V  V  V 

6 

I5H 

1  02 

12-6 

+  +  +  +  v 

+  +  v  +  v 

V  +  +  V  V 

6 

12^ 

106 

12-6 

+  +  +  +  + 

+  +  v  +  + 

V  V  +  V  + 

6 

15^ 

119 

12-8 

+  +  +  +  + 

v  +  v  +  + 

V  V  +  V  V 

6 

12^ 

118 

12-8 

+  +  +  +  + 

v  +  +  +  + 

v  +  +  +  v 

6 

i52A 

H3 

12-8 

+  +  +  +  + 

+  v  v  +  + 

+  +  +  +  + 

6 

A    or    153/5 

108 

12-9 

+  +  v  +  + 

+  +  v  +  + 

V  V  V  V  V 

6 

10    or    n 

103 

12-9 

+  +  +  +  + 

+  +  v  +  v 

V  V  V  V  V 

6 

I3*i 

1  20 

12-11 

+  +  +  +  + 

+  V  V  +  + 

v  +  +  v  + 

6 

15^ 

99 

13 

+  +  +  +  + 

+  V   V  V  A 

V  +  V  V  V 

6 

12^ 

122 

13 

+  +  +  +  v 

+  V  V  V  + 

v  +  +  v  + 

6 

12^ 

117 

13-4 

+  +  +  +  + 

V  V  V  V  + 

V  +  +  +  V 

6 

l2Ys 

no 

13-5 

+  +  +  +  + 

+  +  v  +  v 

+  +  +  +  v 

6 

IS2/S 

107 

14-2 

+  +  +  +  v 

V  V  V  V  V 

V  +  V  V  V 

6 

12 

78  CLARA  SCHMITT 

The  final  column  of  each  table  shows  the  mental  age  attained  by 
each  individual.  Where  the  ages  of  ten  and  twelve  and  fifteen 
overlap  there  is  some  ambiguity  as  to  the  grading  for  mental  age. 
Should  the  children  who  fail  in  some  of  the  ten  year  tests  but 
make  the  additional  five  in  the  twelve  and  fifteen  sets  be  graded 
eleven  years  or  twelve  years  mentally  ?  The  type  of  performance 
with  the  1 2- 1  test  also  complicates  the  grading.  Where  there 
may  be  an  alternative  grade  the  fact  has  been  indicated  in  an 
additional  column. 

The  results  are  arranged  in  table  VIII  which  shows  the  rela- 
tion of  chronological  age  to  mental  age.  In  this  table  are 
indicated  the  alternative  gradings  and  the  resulting  alternative 
percentages.  The  150  children  grade  14  per  cent  (or  20) 
retarded,  26  per  cent  (or  24)  normal,  and  55  per  cent  (or  54) 
advanced. 

Table  VIII 

Re-        Nor-       Ad- 
tarded       mal      vanced 


Chronc 
ical  a 

Mental  Age 

u 

.0 

.0 

3 
& 

& 

.0 

§ 

55 

& 

.a 

a 

3 

55 

£ 

)log-    / 
ge       678 

9    10      ii 

12      15 

A'.I 

5  to 

5-6    2     i 

3 

O 

o 

o 

o 

3 

IOO 

5-6  to 

6-6374 

H 

O 

o 

3 

21 

II 

78 

6-6  to 

7-6    i     3    8 

6 

18 

I 

5 

3 

16 

14 

77 

7-6  to 

8-6          i     3 

II      IO 

25 

I 

4 

3 

12 

21 

84 

8-6  to 

9-6 

3     18 

21 

O 

o 

3 

14 

18 

85 

4 

9-6  to 

10-6 

or 

19     [3] 

i 

23 

o 

0 

19 

82 

4 

17 

5 

6 

5 

45 

o 

O 

6 

54 

io-6  to 

n-6 

or 

or 

or 

or 

or 

7     [4] 

0 

II 

7 

63 

4 

36 

o 

o 

o 

9 

o 

o 

9 

56 

1  1-6  to 

12-6 

or 

or 

or 

or 

i     [51 

3      6 

i     16 

6 

38 

3 

18 

7 

43 

2 

9      4 

i 

12-6  to 

13-6 

or 

or    or 

or 

i      3     [3l 

5      5 

o    17 

12 

70 

O 

o 

5 

29 

13-6  to 

14-6 

2 

2 

2 

IOO 

Total 

ISO 

21 

14 

40 

26 

88 

Is 

or 

or 

or 

or 

30 

20 

37 

24 

81 

54 

Showing  the  relation  of  chronological  to  mental  age  of  150  normal  children. 


STANDARDIZATION  OF  TESTS  79 

With  these  results  we  may  compare  those  of  table  IX,  which 
shows  the  relation  of  grade  to  chronological  age.  The  numbers 
in  heavy  type  mark  those  of  normal  age  for  the  grade.  Since 
the  data  was  obtained  at  the  end  of  the  school  year  the  normal 
age  for  the  kindergarten  is  that  of  the  beginning  first  grade,  for 
the  first  grade  that  of  the  beginning  second  grade,  etc. — that  is, 
the  kindergarten  children  were  ready  for  the  first  grade  and 
became  first  grade  children  at  the  beginning  of  the  next  year. 
The  table  shows  that  38  per  cent  are  retarded,  56  per  cent 
normal  and  4  per  cent  advanced  with  respect  to  their  school 
work  as  compared  with  the  14  per  cent,  26  per  cent  and  58 
per  cent  respectively  with  respect  to  Binet-Simon  mental  age. 

TABLE  IX 
Age  Grade  Correlation 

Re-        Nor-      Ad- 
Chronological  Age  tarded      mal      vanced 


VO 

S1 

s> 

V 

/  

—  \ 

r     '  •" 

N 

r— 

—  \ 

£ 

VO 

oo 

* 

O 

h-t 

5 

<M 

r? 

M 

hi 

IH 

t-, 

T^ 

o 

o 

Q 

O           4J 

o 

<U 

0) 

VO         O 

o 

O 

O 

—       .Q 

^ 

^Q 

_<*> 

o 

VO 

VO 

VO 

vo        5 

E 

s 

E 

+J         VO 

»3 

^ 

Grade 

IT)        VO 

^ 

00 

ON 

O 

t—  i 

O) 

»—  1 

I-H             Z 

55 

& 

& 

S5 

*& 

Kdg. 

17      6 

2 

25 

8 

32 

17 

68 

o 

o 

I 

12 

7 

I 

20 

8 

40 

12 

60 

o 

o 

II 

12 

5 

17 

5 

29 

12 

70 

0 

o 

III 

2 

13 

6 

21' 

6 

28 

13 

61 

2 

9 

IV 

2 

14 

4 

2 

22 

6 

27 

14 

63 

2 

9 

V 

3 

7 

6 

5 

I      22 

12 

54 

7 

3 

14 

VI 

10 

12 

I      23 

13 

56 

10 

43 

0 

o 

Total  150    58    38    85    56      7      4 

Showing  relation  of  grade  to  chronological  age  of  150  normal  children. 

Table  X  shows  the  relation  of  school  grade  to  the  mental 
age  grading  of  the  Binet  series.  If  school  grade  age  and  Binet 
age  correspond  the  normal  Binet  age  for  the  Kindergarten 
would  be  six  years,  for  the  first  grade  seven  years,  etc.  The 
normal  mental  age  for  the  grade  is  indicated  by  the  heavy  type. 
The  number  retarded  according  to  the  Binet  age  with  reference 
to  the  normal  grade  age  is  2  per  cent  (or  4)  ;  normal,  25  per 
cent  (or  35);  advanced  72  per  cent  (or  60). 

The  results  of  the  three  preceding  tables  arrange  themselves 
as  follows : 


80  CLARA  SCHMITT 

Retarded  Normal  Advanced 

14  (or  20)  %  26  (or  24)  %   58  (or  54)  %    By    Binet    Age    to    Chronological 

Age. 
38  36  4  By  School  Grade  to  Chronological 

Age. 
2  (or  4)          25  (or  35)        72  (or  60)         By  Binet  Age  to  School  Grade  Age. 

These  figures  show  the  wide  variance  in  the  various  gradings. 
Where  the  school  grading  shows  4  per  cent  advanced  over  the 
normal  for  the  chronological  age,  the  Binet  grading  shows  58 
per  cent  over  the  chronological  age  and  72  per  cent  over  the 
age  normal  to  the  school  grade. 

TABLE  X 

Re-        Nor-       Ad- 
tarded        mal      vanced 


u 

1 

O 

Mental  Age 

| 

| 

* 

| 

* 

1 

3 
fe 

* 

6   7   8   9  10  ii 

12  15   A. 

Kdg. 

6  ii   8 

25 

0 

o 

6 

24 

19 

76 

I 

0   7   II    2 

20 

O 

0 

o 

0 

2O 

100 

II 

o   7  10 

17 

O 

0 

o 

o 

17 

IOO 

I 

III 

i  19  [i] 

or 

21 

o 

o 

i 

4 

2O 

95 

o 

IV 

21 

i 

22 

o 

o 

21 

95 

I 

4 

2 

19 

3 

13 

O 

o 

19 

86 

V 

I  or 

or 

or 

or 

or 

or 

or 

or 

5  [15] 

i 

22 

6 

26 

15 

68 

I 

4 

i 

IO   2 

VI 

or  [i] 

10   or  or 

o 

ir   i 

23 

i 

4 

IO 

43 

12 

52 

Total  150      4      2    38    25  108      72 

or    or    or    or    or      or 
7      4    53    35    90      60 

Showing  relation  of  normal  grade  age  to  Binet  mental  age  of  150  normal 
children. 

Table  X  when  analyzed  further  shows  that  below  the  fourth 
grade  the  Binet  tests  are  not  suited  to  children  with  the  educa- 
tional experiences  of  those  grades,  since  from  76  per  cent  to 
100  per  cent  are  advanced  by  the  Binet  tests  over  the  age  normal 
to  the  school  grade.  At  the  fourth  grade  the  age  normal  to 
school  grade  and  the  Binet  age  correspond  very  closely.  At  the 
fifth  grade  the  Binet  age  is  advanced  or  rather  close  to  the 
normal  according  to  the  convention  chosen  for  the  Binet  grad- 
ing. At  the  sixth  grade  it  is  rather  evenly  divided  between 
advanced  and  normal. 


VI 

STANDARDIZATION  AND  DISCUSSION  OF  THE 
HEALY-FERNALD  TESTS 

Origin  of  the  Healy-Fernald  tests. — The  Healy-Fernald  set 
of  tests  was  worked  out  for  two  purposes.  The  first  was  for 
the  purpose  of  supplementing  the  Binet-Simon  series.  It  was 
discovered  in  the  work  of  the  clinic  that  the  Binet-Simon  series 
failed  to  explore  with  sufficient  thoroughness  the  mentality  of  the 
child.  The  objections  to  the  Binet-Simon  series  have  been 
discussed  a1x>ve. 

The  second  reason  for  the  preparation  of  the  new  tests  arose 
from  the  language  difficulty  which  confronted  the  workers  of 
the  clinic.  Many  children  who  come  to  the  clinic  are  of  foreign 
parentage  or  from  homes  where  a  foreign  language  is  spoken. 
In  many  cases  they  come  from  parochial  schools  where  little 
English  is  taught  or  used.  In  other  cases,  especially  those  of 
rather  young  children,  the  reactions  to  the  Binet-Simon  tests 
were  of  doubtful  significance,  because  the  tester  could  not  be 
sure  that  the  language  in  which  these  tests  were  given  was 
completely  understood  by  the  child.  The  difficulty  of  using  the 
English  language  with  a  child  from  an  alien  speaking  home 
can  be  appreciated  only  by  one  who  has  mingled  with  these 
children  informally  on  their  own  ground.  The  child  confronts 
a  situation  in  which  he  has  not  only  two  languages  with  which 
to  deal,  but  rather  three.  There  is  the  foreign  language  in 
the  home,  the  patois  which  he  gets  on  the  street,  and  the 
classical  language  of  the  cultured  person  with  whom  he  comes 
in  contact  at  school  or  other  cultural  institutions.  The  street 
patois  is  surely  not  an  unimportant  factor  in  causing  mental 
confusion  in  the  mind  of  the  child.  In  the  realm  of  patois  a 
man  is  never  a  man,  he  is  a  guy;  a  boy  is  never  a  boy,  he  is  a  kid; 
?.  foolish  person  is  never  such,  he  is  a  mut;  one  never  stops  doing 
something,  or  is  commanded  to  leave  off  doing  something,  he 


82  CLARA  SCHMITT 

must  always  cheese  it;  and  so  on  interminably.  The  confusion 
can  well  be  imagined  in  the  mind  of  the  child  who,  at  home,  is  told 
in  a  foreign  language  by  his  mother  to  leave  off  doing  some- 
thing; who  by  his  older  brothers  or  street  companions  is  com- 
manded to  cheese  it;  and  who  by  his  teacher  or  other  cultured 
person  with  whom  he  comes  in  contact,  is  in  more  or  less 
classical  terms  requested  to  stop  his  misconduct.  If  the  child 
has  never  attended  a  public  school,  but  only  a  parochial  school 
in  which  English  is  very  little  taught,  the  difficulty  of  examina- 
tion with  such  a  series  of  tests  as  the  Binet-Simon,  which 
makes  use  of  language  almost  wholly,  can  well  be  imagined. 
For  these  reasons,  tests  which  show  the  functioning  of  intelli- 
gence without  the  necessity  of  accuracy  in  the  use  of  language 
were  originated  by  the  workers  and  friends  of  the  Psychopathic 
clinic. 

Evaluation  of  Results. — In  the  attempt  to  express  the  dif- 
ference between  the  defective  and  the  normal  human  intellect 
one  is  confronted  by  two  possibilities.  The  first  is,  that  there 
exists  a  distinguishable  qualitative  difference.  This  idea  was  most 
vividly  expressed  by  Tregold  when  he  said  that  there  exists  be- 
tween the  highest  ament  and  the  lowest  normal  individual  an  im- 
passable gulf.  The  qualitative  factor  of  difference  has  been 
discussed  with  reference  to  the  application  of  the  Binet-Simon 
tests  to  the  task  of  distinguishing  between  the  normal  and  the 
defective.  The  second  possibility  is  that  the  difference  is  only 
a  quantitative  one.  It  is  to  the  effect  that  there  exists  a  normal 
curve  of  distribution  of  mental  abilities  corresponding  to  the 
theoretically  normal  curve  to  be  obtained  from  a  large  mass 
of  fine  measurements.  Notwithstanding  the  theory  of  quali- 
tative discrimination  underlying  its  origin  the  attempt  has  been 
made  by  Goddard,  Kuhlman,  Chotzen  and  others  to  fit  the 
Binet-Simon  series  into  this  conception.  Their  theory  of  the 
difference  between  the  normal  and  the  defective  is  that  the 
latter  takes  more  time  chronologically  to  reach  a  certain  point 
of  development  than  does  the  former.  Clinical  experience  in 
getting  developmental  histories  of  defectives  goes  to  show  that 
in  many  phases  of  development  that  is  the  case.  Defective  chil- 


STANDARDIZATION  OF  TESTS  83 

dren  learn  to  walk  and  talk  later  than  normal  children.  In 
matters  of  formal  education  they  acquire  more  slowly.  In 
the  clinic  which  has  for  its  task  the  classification  of  children  for 
educational,  penal  or  other  practical  social  purposes  this  type 
of  data  is  inadequate  to  fulfill  the  demands  of  the  situation. 
Developmental  history  can  not  always  be  obtained  with  ac- 
curacy; there  are  many  causes  of  retardation  of  the  develop- 
mental phases  of  early  life  the  effects  of  which  do  not  persist 
to  a  later  period;  progress  in  formal  education  and  acquiring 
of  information  may  be  interfered  with  by  any  one  or  more 
of  several  physical  and  social  factors. 

This  use  of  the  time  factor  is  only  another  application  of 
the  quantitative  idea  of  difference  discussed  in  the  first  chapter. 
The  use  of  the  time  factor  whether  in  the  sense  oif  marking 
off  developmental  ranges  of  difference,  or  whether  applied  in 
the  rigorous  laboratory  method  to  specific  tests  can  make  the 
point  of  distinction  between  the  normal  and  the  defective  only 
an  arbitrary  matter.  With  the  curve  of  normal  distribution 
of  quantitatively  measurable  phases  of  mental  processes  before 
us  who  is  to  decide  this  determining  point  and  upon  what  basis  ? 

In  order  to  give  a  'further  meaning  and  value  to  the  quanti- 
tative data  obtainable  in  a  clinical  examination  the  writer 
proposes  certain  qualitative  classifications  in  the  discussion  to 
follow. 

The  quantitative  data  used  to  determine  the  qualitative  classi- 
fications to  be  described  below  are,  for  the  most  part,  number 
and  types  of  errors.  The  classifications  are  made  upon  such 
considerations  as  the  relation  of  error  in  individual  cases  to 
the  number  of  errors  possible  to  the  test  to  be  evaluated,  other 
conditions  peculiar  to  the  test  itself,  and  the  object  of  the  test 
from  the  standpoint  of  the  child. 

Some  of  the  reasons  for  the  exclusion  of  time  measure  in 
evaluation  of  results  were  discussed  above.  They  are  inherent 
in  the  demands  oif  the  clinical  situation.  The  motive  to  make  a 
good  time  record  is  unsuited  to  the  practical  demands  of  the 
clinic,  because  it  is  the  desire  there  to  test  for  the  most  part 
such  processes  as  require  attentional  control  in  a  new  situation. 


84  CLARA  SCHMITT 

'For  some  tests  such  as  tapping  tests  and  the  Thorndike  a  test, 
the  time  measure  is  an  important  factor.  In  such  tests  there 
is  no  new  discrimination  in  the  perceptual  or  other  mental  pro- 
cesses to  be  made.  If  other  things,  such  as  the  avoidance  of 
error  or  the  making  of  a  plan  for  a  bit  of  work,  are  of  most 
importance,  time  can  not  except  within  large  limits  be  taken 
into  consideration.  In  the  writer's  proposed  classifications  the 
only  use  of  the  time  factor  is  to  mark  the  point  where  the 
child's  reaction  to  the  test  may  be  classed  as  failure. 

A  further  reason  for  eliminating  time  measure  from  the 
evaluation  of  results  is  that  much  time  may  be  wasted  by  the 
child  who  is  working  from  the  play  motive.  His  attention 
may  be  dften  diverted  from  consideration  of  the  end  of  the 
test.  If  he  stops  to  remark  that  it  is  a  pretty  puzzle,  or  to  ask 
who  made  it,  the  amount  of  time  so  wasted  will  depend  some- 
what upon  the  tact  of  the  examiner  in  again  directing  his 
attention  to  the  work  in  hand.  This,  then,  leads  to  the  further 
consideration  that  one  does  not  know  whether  one  unit  of  time 
has  been  of  the  same  value  as  any  other  unit  in  the  performance 
of  the  test.  If  the  child  takes  some  time  apparently  examining 
the  test  before  him  before  beginning,  or  stops  to  do  so  at  any 
time  during  the  performance  of  it,  one  does  not  know  what  is 
taking  place  in  his  mind.  One  does  not  know  whether  he  is 
examining  it  with  reference  to  the  requirements  of  the  test; 
or  is  occupying  his  attention  with  something  quite  apart  from 
the  object  of  the  test  such  as  the  colors  or  the  grain  of  the 
wood  when  doing  puzzle  tests;  or  is  /only  staring  and  not 
thinking  or  planning.  In  an  examination  one  is  sometimes  at 
a  loss  as  to  how  to  direct  the  attention  of  the  child  because  what 
he  is  really  doing  can  be  often  only  a  matter  of  conjecture. 

It  has  been  shown  in  the  first  chapter  that  laboratory  tests 
suitable  to  such  fine  discriminations  of  measurement  as  is  de- 
manded in  rigorous  laboratory  method  correlate  more  or  less 
doubtfully  with  general  intelligence,  the  matter  to  be  deter- 
mined, or  measured.  The  reason  for  this  is  that  there  exists 
no  measure  of  general  intelligence  which  permits  of  such  fine 
discriminations  as  are  used  in  laboratory  tests.  An  analogous 


STANDARDIZATION  OF  TESTS  85 

process  in  the  realm  of  physics  would  be  the  attempt  to  find  a 
proportion  between  an  object  measured  with  a  micrometer  and 
another  measured  with  a  yard  stick.  The  object  of  the  clinic 
for  the  present  should  be  to  find  such  tests  as  will  present 
suitable  situations  in  which  the  general  intelligence  may  func- 
tion, rather  than  to  find  a  correlate  or  measure  in  simple 
mental  processes.  The  tests  should  be  of  various  grades  of 
complexity,  but  analyzable  with  reference  to  such  standards  as 
we  possess  ;for  judging  human  social  conduct.  It  is  with  the 
belief  that  they  correspond  to  mental  processes  concerned  in 
the  social  adjustment  of  the  individual,  and  in  the  relation  of 
the  individual  to  social  progress,  that  the  writer  proposes  the 
following  qualitative  classifications  of  reaction  to  the  tests  under 
discussion. 

The  three  classifications  chosen  are  termed,  planned  reaction, 
trial  and  error  reaction  and  chance  reaction.  In  the  first  type 
the  subject  applies  his  previous  experiences  of  the  kind  presented 
to  him  by  the  test  to  the  solution  of  the  specific  problem  before 
him,  with  a  minimum  of  error.  In  the  second  type  the  situation 
is  approached  as  though  entirely  new,  in  which  there  is  little 
conscious  application  of  previous  experience  to  the  solution  of 
the  problem  presented,  but  in  which  the  experiences  presented 
by  the  present  problem  become  the  basis  for  attack  upon  a 
new  problem  of  similar  type.  In  the  third  type  of  reaction 
every  new  problem  is  wholly  new,  and  the  experiences  of  early 
attempts  with  it  do  not  become  a  basis  for  conscious  modification 
of  reaction  in  further  work  with  it. 

The  animal  or  low  type  of  human  intellect  arrives  at  new 
attainments  such  as  the  opening  of  a  lock,  through  a  chance 
coordination  gained  after  much  trial  and  error.  Every  new 
lock  must  be  an  entirely  new  problem.  Such  an  intelligence 
does  not  generalize  upon  past  experiences  in  such  way  as  to  bring 
about  an  adaptation  of  the  old  response  to  the  changed  conditions 
of  the  old  type  of  problem. 

In  the  following  tables  may  be  noticed  certain  time  correla- 
tions with  grade  and  in  one  case,  Table  XXI,  with  method 
of  procedure  or  type  of  reaction.  Since,  however,  the  time  de- 


86  CLARA  SCHMITT 

creases  with  the  higher  grades  for  both  types  of  reaction,  the 
decrease  has  no  relation  to  the  mental  process.  It  probably 
indicates  increase  of  motor  ability  with  the  higher  grades. 

General  Methods  of  Procedure  in  Giving  Tests. — The  private 
school  children  to  whom  the  tests  were  given  were  told  by  the 
principal  that  we  had  some  games  with  which  they  might  play, 
coming  one  at  a  time,  and  that  we  wanted  to  see  how  well 
they  liked  them.  They  were  told  not  to  tell  their  mates  about 
the  games  after  having  played  with  them,  for  it  would  spoil 
the  fun  of  those  who  were  still  to  see  them.  This  way  of 
putting  the  matter  was  very  effective,  for  the  children  who 
had  not  yet  had  the  tests  would  not  permit  those  who  had  to 
discuss  them.  An  effort  was  made  to  send  each  child  back  to 
his  school  room  with  the  feeling  that  he  had  had  a  very  good 
time.  The  other  children  then  came  with  only  pleasant  antici- 
pations. The  children  were  given  the  tests  singly  in  a  quiet 
room  with  which  they  were  familiar.  The  word  quiet  is  not 
intended  to  convey  the  idea  that  it  was  noiseless.  As  a  matter 
of  fact  it  was  on  a  street  car  line  and  many  other  noises  such 
as  the  closing  of  doors,  etc.,  reached  it.  The  noises  were  such, 
however,  as  the  children  were  accustomed  to  hearing  and  did 
not  distract  attention. 

The  older  children  who  were  not  satisfied  with  the  reason 
for  giving  the  tests  were  told  that  we  wanted  to  see  how  much 
better  older  children  could  play  the  games  than  younger  ones. 
Very  few  of  the  children  asked  for  a  reason  further  than  the 
one  given  them  by  the  principal,  that  the  games  were  intended 
for  their  own  amusement.  In  the  clinic  it  has  been  found  that 
this  reason  generally  suffices.  In  case  the  real  reason  is  de- 
manded it  is  generally  best  to  give  one  that  assures  the  child 
of  the  examiner's  personal  friendship  toward  him.  One  to  the 
effect  that  we  want  to  see  how  well  he  can  do  these  things  so 
that  we  may  know  what  kind  of  work  to  get  for  him  or  how 
to  help  him  out  of  his  trouble,  if  coupled  with  the  assurance 
all  along  that  he  is  doing  well,  is  always  satisfactory. 

Test  I.  Introductory  Picture  Form  Board. — This  test,  with 
the  exception  of  the  sixth  grade,  was  always  given  first.  Its 


FIG.  i 


PLATE  I 

A  PICTURE  FORM-BOARD — OUR  TEST  I 

An  example  of  a  test  in  which  form  and  color  perceptions,  some  apperceptions, 
and  methods  of  trial  and  success  are  brought  out. 

Prom   INDIVIDUAL  DELINQUENTS — HEALY 

Courtesy  Little,  Brown  &r  Co.  , 


STANDARDIZATION  OF  TESTS  87 

bright  colors  and  the  animal  pictures  immediately  excite  interest. 
Its  simplicity  disarms  suspicion  that  anything  unusual  or  difficult 
will  be  required.  The  pleasure  of  constructing  the  puzzle  leaves 
the  child  in  a  very  pleasant  and  anticipatory  frame  of  mind 
for  further  tests.  By  giving  tests  of  this  character  first  the 
child's  friendliness  for,  and  confidence  in  the  examiner  grows 
to  such  an  extent  that  he  is  willing  to  undertake  tests  of  a 
less  immediately  interesting  character. 

The  design  shows  a  certain  number  of  pieces  cut  out  on 
the  natural  lines  of  some  of  the  objects  in  the  picture,  together 
with  four  other  pieces,  one  of  which  is  irregular  in  shape,  and 
three  of  which  are  cut  on  geometrical  lines.  Two  of  these  last 
somewhat  resemble  each  other,  but  are  not  interchangeable.  The 
third  is  an  isosceles  triangle  divided  into  two  right  angle 
triangles.  The  purpose  of  this  was  to  provide  for  a  simple 
trial  and  error  procedure,  if  the  make-up  of  the  parent  triangle 
was  not  at  once  recognized — as  it  usually  is  not. 

The  puzzle  is  put  before  the  child,  the  pieces  scattered  at 
random  on  the  table,  with  the  remark  that  here  is  a  pretty 
puzzle  which  one  would  like  to  see  how  well  he  can  do.  With 
the  exception  of  the  divided  triangle  this  test  presents  a  one 
to  one  relationship  between  the  openings  and  the  pieces  to  be 
placed,  that  is  there  is  one  opening  for  each  piece.  The  usual 
procedure  is  to  leave  the  divided  triangle  until  the  last,  since 
there  is  no  place  for  either  piece  alone.  The  high  grade  child 
above  five  years  of  age  makes  few  errors  or  none  at  all  in 
placing  pieces  other  than  those  of  the  divided  triangle.  That 
is,  he  does  not  attempt  to  put  a  piece  where  it  does  not  belong, 
the  head  where  the  legs  ought  to  be,  etc.  The  child  of  low 
grade  intelligence  places  the  pieces  by  trial  and  error,  trying 
each  piece  in  each  opening  until  he  finds  the  one  in  which  it 
fits.  The  still  lower  grade  of  intelligence  persistently  tries  to 
fit  a  piece  into  an  opening  and  finally  fails  of  accomplishing  the 
test  because  his  lack  of  recognition  of  failure  has  prevented 
his  giving  up  a  misfit  attempt  in  favor  of  another  opening  for 
the  piece  he  is  trying.  The  divided  triangle,  because  of  the 


88  CLARA  SCHMITT 

difference  between  the  numerical  relation  of  the  opening  and 
the  pieces,  really  constitutes  a  test  within  itself.  It  may  be 
placed  without  any  trial  and  error,  in  which  case  the  subject 
immediately  perceives  the  relation  between  the  opening  and 
the  pieces.  It  may  be  placed  by  trial  and  error  in  which  the 
pieces  are  turned  around  and  around  until  the  right  position 
is  hit  upon.  The  most  common  error  of  the  child  in  this  re- 
peated trial  and  error  is  to  turn  the  piece  through  an  arc  of 
1 80°  or  more  instead  of  through  90°  which  would  accomplish 
the  task.  Because  of  the  diversity  between  the  body  of  the 
puzzle  and  the  triangular  portion  these  two  parts  are  tabulated 
as  two  distinct  tests.  Table  XI  presents  the  data  obtained  from 
the  body  of  the  puzzle. 

TABLE  XI 

Introductory  Puzzle.     Test  I.     (Body  of  Puzzle) 

Errors 


3  to  5         6  or  more 
o  errors     i  &  2  errors     errors  errors 

Grade  Number  Time    Number  %  Number  %  Number  %  Number  % 


Kdg. 

27 

I  '-41" 

9 

33 

7 

25 

8 

29 

3 

II 

I 

21 

l'-2l" 

16 

76 

3 

14 

2 

9 

o 

o 

II 

17 

l'-l6" 

9 

52 

5 

29 

3 

17 

o 

o 

III 

21 

i'-  4" 

10 

47 

7 

33 

4 

19 

o 

o 

IV 

24 

i'-  3" 

13 

54 

ii 

45 

o 

o 

o 

o 

V 

22 

I'-IQ" 

12 

54 

9 

40 

i 

4 

o 

o 

The  table  shows  the  average  time  of  performance  of  the 
children  of  the  different  grades,  and  the  number  and  per  cent 
of  errors  which  are  indicated  in  the  fourth  and  succeeding 
columns  to  the  left.  An  error  is  any  wrong  attempt  to  place  a 
piece.  It  is  counted  an  error  to  take  any  piece  and  attempt  to 
place  it  in  an  opening  other  than  the  one  in  which  it  fits;  if  the 
piece  is  turned  about  and  placed  in  another  wrong  opening 
another  error  is  scored;  if  the  attempt  is  made  to  place  a  piece 
in  its  own  opening  upside  down  an  error  is  scored;  if  a  piece 
is  discarded  and  later  tried  again  in  the  same  wrong  opening 
an  additional  error  is  scored  for  the  second  and  each  succeed- 
ing wrong  attempt.  The  table  shows  that  the  trial  and  error 
is  small  after  the  kindergarten,  when  80  per  cent  or  more  of 


STANDARDIZATION  OF  TESTS  89 

each  grade  perform  the  puzzle  with  less  than  three  errors; 
89  per  cent  of  the  kindergarten  children  make  less  than  six 
errors.  The  most  common  errors  are  the  attempts  to  inter- 
change the  two  heads,  the  two  legs,  and  the  diamond  and 
modified  diamond.  The  errors  of  normal  children  are  attempts 
to  interchange  these  roughly  paired  pieces.  Defective  children 
will  attempt  such  errors  as  placing  a  head  where  a  leg  should 
be,  etc. 

Table  XII  presents  the  data  obtained  from  the  triangular 
portion  of  the  test.  The  data  are  arranged  to  show  the  average 
time  required  to  arrange  the  two  pieces  in  the  opening,  and 
the  type  of  mental  process  which  the  child  employed  in  ac- 
complishing the  task.  The  data  have  been  arranged  to  show  two 
types  or  methods  of  work,  the  trial  and  error  method  and  the 
planned  method.  A  child  was  recorded  in  the  trial  and  error 
column  if  he  tried  each  piece  in  more  than  two  positions  before 
finding  the  right  one,  and  in  the  planned  column  if  he  tried 
one  or  both  pieces  in  less  than  two  positions  before  finding 
the  right  one.  A  more  careful  grading  of  the  trial  and  error 
method  is  not  practicable  because  the  pieces  are  turned  quickly 
and  often  held  covered  up. 

TABLE  XII 

Introductory   Puzzle,   Test   I    (Triangle) 

Method 


Average 

Failure 

Trial  & 

error 

Planned 

f 

> 

t 

A 

/ 

\ 

Grade 

Number 

time 

Number      % 

Number        %       Number      % 

Kdg 

27 

i  '-36" 

4 

14 

23 

100 

0 

o 

I 

21 

i  '-28" 

i 

4 

15 

76 

5 

23 

II 

17 

i  '-25" 

i 

5 

12 

70 

5 

29 

III 

21 

i  '-25" 

o 

o 

12 

57 

9 

42 

IV 

24 

54" 

o 

0 

21 

88 

3 

ii 

V 

22 

33" 

0 

o 

13 

59 

9 

40 

The  percentage  of  trial  and  error  of  the  above  table  is  that 
of  the  sum  of  the  trial  and  error  and  the  failure  records.  The 
object  is  to  find  the  percentage  of  children  who  do  not  do  the 
test  by  a  method  superior  to  that  of  trial  and  error.  It  is 
probable  that  those  marked  failure,  if  permitted  to  work  in- 
definitely, would  accomplish  the  task.  The  table  shows  that 


go  CLARA  SCHMITT 

from  60  per  cent  to  100  per  cent  of  the  children  between  the 
kindergarten  and  the  fifth  grade  have  not  had  sufficient  ex- 
perience With  such  geometrical  problems  to  enable  them  to  do 
this  one  without  trial  arid  error.  Since,  then,  the  problem 
cannot  be  used  as  a  test  of  a  child's  ability  to  perceive  the 
spatial  relationship  involved  without  error,  it  may  be  used  as 
a  test  of  his  ability  to  learn  by  experience  with  it.  Twenty-six 
of  the  kindergarten  children  were  asked  to  do  the  triangle  a 
second  time.  Those  who  had  failed  in  five  minutes  were  shown 
how  to  do  it.  Of  these  twenty-six  children,  twenty  did  it  a 
second  time  without  error,  requiring,  with  one  exception,  not 
more  than  twenty  seconds;  three  children  did  it  a  second  time 
with  repeated  trial  and  error,  but  the  third  time  without  error; 
and  three  did  it  a  second  time  with  error  but  no  repeated  error, 
that  is  no  wrong  position  for  each  piece  was  tried  more  than 
once.  These  results  may  be  compared  with  those  obtained  from 
delinquent  children  seen  at  the  Juvenile  Court  Clinic  mentioned 
above. 

Of  twenty-six  children  between  7-6  and  8-6  years  of  age 
seen  at  the  clinic,  five  because  of  their  reactions  to  this  and 
other  tests,  were  graded  feeble-minded.  Of  these  five  cases, 
three  failed  to  complete  the  body  of  the  puzzle  because  of  lack 
of  recognition  of  failure  in  attempting  to  place  the  pieces; 
two  others  failed  to  complete  the  triangle.  Among  those  graded 
normal  there  were  no  failures  of  either  part  of  the  puzzle;  four 
of  the  normal  children  made  six  or  more  errors  in  doing  the 
body  of  the  puzzle,  four  did  the  triangle  by  trial  and  error. 
Of  twenty-.two  children  between  the  ages  of  8-6  and  9-6  years, 
three  were  graded  feeble-minded.  Two  of  these  children  ac- 
complished the  whole  puzzle,  with  more  than  six  errors  for 
the  body  and  the  triangle  by  trial  and  error;  the  third  failed 
on  the  triangle.  Of  the  children  graded  normal,  one  failed 
to  accomplish  the  puzzle,  one  made  six  errors,  and  two  did  the 
triangle  by  trial  and  error,  and  one  failed  to  do  the  triangle.* 

*  In  the  process  of  evaluating  a  child's  mental  condition,  in  general,  failure 
with  one  test  which  the  child's  age  might  lead  one  to  expect  him  to  accom- 
plish is  disregarded  if  he  has  uniformly  accomplished  more  complex  tests. 
One  must  take  into  consideration  the  fact  that  clinical  conditions  can  not 


o^o 


FIG.  2 


STANDARDIZATION  OF  TESTS  gi 

Above  this  age  failure  in  one  or  both  parts  of  this  test  always 
accompanies  the  condition  jof  .feeble-mindedness,  but  not  all 
feeble-minded  persons  above  this  age  fail  in  the  test. 

Test  II.  Special  Picture  Puzzle.  This  test  was  given  second 
in  order  to  the  first  four  grades.  The  twelve  pieces  of  the 
puzzle  are  so  cut  as  to  form  five  rather  closely  resembling 
pairs  and  two  unpaired  pieces.  Four  of  the  pairs  differ  in 
shape  and  cannot  be  interchangeably  placed.  The  quadrilateral 
pieces  can  be  interchanged. 

Except  for  the  two  unpaired  pieces  the  differences  of  form 
are  not  sufficient  to  serve  as  a  guide  in  placing  the  pieces.  The 
placing  must  be  accomplished  by  the  matching  of  the  lines  and 
colors  preserved  on  the  piece  with  those  of  the  surrounding 
picture. 

In  giving  the  test  the  form  is  placed  before  the  child  with 
the  pieces  scattered  at  random.  He  is  told  that  "the  game" 
with  this  puzzle  is  to  look  so  carefully  at  each  piece  before  at- 
tempting to  place  it  that  he  will  not  try  to  put  any  piece  where 
it  does  not  belong;  that  is,  that  he  should  not  try  to  give  any- 
one the  wrong  head,  but  give  to  each  one  just  what  belongs 
to  him  at  the  very  first  trial.  If  the  child  makes  an  error  in 
attempting  the  first  piece  the  warning  is  again  repeated  with 
the  remark  that  he  has  just  made  a  mistake  and  should  look 
carefully  and  not  do  it  again.  With  young  children  the  author 
has  tried  to  excite  greater  interest  in  doing  the  puzzle  care- 
fully by  saying  that  to  try  to  give  any  boy  the  wrong  head 
hurts  him  very  much  and  care  should  be  taken  not  to  do  that. 
This  extra  appeal  to  the  imagination,  however,  while  amusing 
the  child,  does  not  seem  to  stimulate  him  to  greater  care  in 
placing  the  pieces.  Apparently  the  type  of  motive  for  doing 
the  puzzle,  whether  the  humanitarian  one  just  referred  to  or 
the  play  motive  or  the  desire  to  please  the  examiner  who  has 
asked  him  to  do  it,  has  no  effect  upon  calling  into  greater 
activity  his  ability  to  do  it. 

In  the  performance  of  this  test  account  is  kept  of  time  and 

always  be  kept  uniform  with  reference  to  the  child's  motives,  and  that  the 
child's  reactions  are  not  always  uniform  with  reference  to  his  general 
mental  level. 


92  CLARA  SCHMITT 

errors.  Attempts  to  place  a  piece  in  the  wrong  opening,  to 
place  one  upsidedown  in  its  own  or  a  wrong  opening,  at- 
tempts to  interchange  the  quadrilateral  pieces,  are  counted  as 
errors. 

The  puzzle  may  be  done  by  trial  and  error  in  which  each 
piece  is  tried  out  in  several  places  until  the  right  one  is  found 
for  it.  In  this  case  the  child  does  not  discover  that  some 
other  distinction  than  form  is  necessary  to  aid  him  in  placing 
the  pieces,  and  his  apparent  compliance  with  the  direction  to 
look  carefully  at  each  piece  and  the  opening  before  attempting 
to  place  it  does  not  lead  him  to  see  the  distinctive  differences 
of  color  and  matching  of  pattern  which  would  accomplish  the 
task.  The  mentally  low  grade  child  does  the  test  by  the  trial 
and  error  method. 

Table  XIII  shows  the  data  obtained  from  the  first  four  grades 
and  the  kindergarten. 

TABLE  XIII 
Test  II.     Special  Picture  Puzzle 

Errors 


ii  or 
i  and  2       3  to  5       6  to  10        more 


Grade 

Numbe 

bfl  **             <u 

§•1           "1 
>                   3 

*R 

Numbe 

* 

Numbe 

* 

<u 
,5 

3 

£ 

3 

« 

Kdg. 

II 
I'll 
IV 

22 

21 

17 
21 
21 

3'-i5"        o 

2-8"           I 
2'-Il"           3 

2'-i7"        3 

2'-22"           7 

O 

4 
17 
14 

33 

i 

4 
4 
7 

10 

5 
19 
23 
33 

47 

5 
9 
6 
7 
3 

22 

42 

35 
33 

14 

9 

5 
4 
4 
i 

40 
23 
23 
19 
4 

7 

2 
O 
O 
0 

31 

9 
o 

0 

o 

This  table  shows  that  at  the  fourth  grade  the  perceptive 
abilities  of  these  children  are  such  as  to  lead  them  to  see 
the  distinctions  necessary  for  the  accomplishment  of  the  test 
with  little  error.  At  this  grade  80  per  cent  of  the  children 
are  able  to  do  the  test  with  less  than  three  errors.  Of  the 
kindergarten  children  71  per  cent  do  it  with  six  or  more  errors, 
an  average  of  more  than  one  error  for  each  of  the  five  pairs. 

Of  the  fourteen  feeble-minded  children  between  eleven  and 


ejx' 


FIG.  3 


PLATE  II 

CONSTRUCTION  TEST  A 

An  example  of  a  test  which  demonstrates  planfulness  and  the  powers  of 

learning  by  experience.     The  illustration  shows  the  test  as 

presented,  as  completed,  and  two  types  of  error. 


From   INDIVIDUAL  DELINQUENTS — HEALY 


STANDARDIZATION  OF  TESTS  93 

thirteen  years  of  age  seen  at  the  clinic,  one  did  the  test  with 
two  errors,  two  with  five  errors,  nine  with  from  six  to  twenty 
errors,  and  two  were  of  so  low  grade  as  to  be  unable  to 
attempt  the  test. 

Table  XIV  shows  the  results  of  the  test  arranged  accord- 
ing to  age.     This  table  shows  that  at  the  age  of  9-6  years  75 

TABLE  XIV 

Test  II.     Special  Picture  Puzzle.     (By  age) 

Errors 


ii  or 

i  and  2        3  to  5       6  to  10       more 


Age 

1 

P 

|l 

ja 

P 

* 

.Q 

p 

* 

"1 

* 

"1 

* 

.Q 

E 
1 

* 

5-  to  6-6 
6-6  to  7-6 
7-6  to  8-6 
8-6  to  9-6 
9-6  to  10-6 
10-6  to  n-6 

16 

13 
27 
23 
20 
6 

3'-  9" 

2'-i7"H 

i  -18" 

O 
2 

3 

2 

6 

2 

O 

15 

II 
8 
30 
33 

0 

4 
3 
8 

9 

4 

0 

30 

ii 
34 
45 
66 

2 

4 
IO 
10 

4 

12 
30 

37 

43 

20 

7 

2 

9 
3 

i 

43 
15 
33 
13 
5 

7 

i 

2 
0 
O 

43 
7 
7 
o 
o 

per  cent  of  the  children  do  the  test  with  less  than  three  errors, 
a  marked  advance  over  the  preceding  year  when  42  per  cent 
make  as  good  a  record. 

Test  III.  Construction  Puzzle  A.  This  puzzle  is  made  up 
of  an  outer  frame  and  five  pieces,  two  of  which  are  identical 
in  size  and  shape,  which  fill  up  the  frame  opening  when  properly 
placed.  The  test  may  be  accomplished  with  a  minimum  of  5 
moves,  one  for  each  piece.  There  are  eleven  possible  errors 
without  repetition.  In  giving  the  test,  records  of  the  number 
of  errors  and  the  time  for  its  accomplishment  are  kept.  In  the 
final  evaluation  of  results  the  removal  of  a  piece  from  a  right 
position  is  counted  as  error. 

The  frame  is  placed  before  the  child  with  the  pieces  scattered 
on  the  table  beside  it  and  he  is  told  that  the  pieces  will  exactly 
fill  the  frame  if  he  finds  the  right  way  to  put  them  in.  The 
result  is  counted  failure  if  the  task  is  not  accomplished  in  ten 
minutes. 


94  CLARA  SCHMITT 

Table  XV  shows  the  results  of  the  test  arranged  according 
to  grade. 

TABLE  XV 

Test  III.     Construction  Puzzle  A.     (By  grade) 

Errors 


12  or 
Failure  o  i  to  5      6  to  n        more 


Grade 

Jo 

a 

3 

il 

1 

3 
fc 

* 

1 

3 

* 

1 

3 

* 

I 

3 
fe 

* 

H 

3 
* 

* 

Kdg. 

26 

3'-io" 

9 

34 

I 

3 

5 

19 

6 

23 

5 

19 

I 

2O 

2'-l8" 

5 

25 

I 

5 

4 

20 

4 

20 

6 

30 

II 

17 

2'-  7" 

o 

o 

O 

o 

7 

41 

5 

29 

5 

29 

III 

21 

I  '-34" 

0 

o 

I 

4 

10 

47 

6 

28 

4 

19 

IV 

24 

54" 

0 

o 

3 

12 

15 

62 

6 

25 

o 

0 

V 

22 

i'-  6" 

o 

o 

i 

4 

15 

67 

i 

4 

5 

22 

VI 

24 

43" 

o 

o 

3 

12 

13 

52 

6 

25 

2 

8 

This  table  shows  that  the  number  of  errors  decreases  until 
in  the  fourth  grade  74  per  cent  of  the  children  do  the  test 
with  less  than  half  the  possible  number  of  errors  and  may 
be  considered  as  having  planned  the  disposition  of  the  pieces 
of  the  puzzle.  Those  in  the  6  to  n  error  column  have  made 
more  than  half  the  possible  number  of  unrepeated  errors  and 
may  be  classed  as  having  done  the  test  by  the  method  of 
trial  and  error.  Those  of  the  12  error  and  the  failure  columns 
have  failed  to  learn  from  the  trial  and  error  of  their  attempts 
and  have  repeated  one  or  more  errors.  When  this  repetition 
begins  in  the  child's  performance  the  accomplishment  of  the 
test  is  then  a  matter  of  chance, — that  is,  the  chance  that  he 
will  hit  upon  the  right  relationship  of  the  pieces.  It  is  possible 
and  probable  that  at  some  place  in  this  repeated  trial  and  error, 
learning  and  planning  begin  in  the  case  of  some  children;  but 
where  they  begin  in  any  case  can  be  only  a  matter  of  con- 
jecture on  the  part  of  the  experimenter. 

The  data  of  this  table  have  been  rearranged  in  table  XVI 
to  show  the  percentages  of  planned,  trial  and  error  and  chance 
methods  in  the  accomplishment  of  the  test. 


STANDARDIZATION  OF  TESTS 

TABLE  XVI 

Test  III.     Construction  Puzzle  A 
Method 


95 


Planned 

Trial  and 

Error 

Chance 

Grade 

Number 

Number 

% 

Number 

% 

Number 

% 

Kdg. 
I 
II 
III 
IV 
V 
VI 

26 

20 

17 
21 
24 
22 
24 

6 

5 
7 
ii 
18 
16 
16 

23 
25 
4i 
52 
75 
73 
66 

6 
4 
5 
6 
6 
i 
6 

23 

20 

29 
28 

25 

4 
25 

14 
ii 

5 
4 
o 

5 

2 

53 
55 
29 
19 
O 
22 
8 

This  table  shows  the  increase  of  ability  to  plan  the  work 
of  this  test  up  to  the  fourth  grade  where  it  is  highest,  75 
per  cent.  There  is  a  decrease  of  the  chance  method  of  per- 
formance to  the  fourth  grade  where  it  is  the  lowest. 

Table  XVII  shows  the  same  data  arranged  with  reference 
to  age  and  in  accordance  with  the  plan  of  evaluation  of  table 
XVI. 

TABLE  XVII 

Test  III.     Construction  Puzzle  A.     (By  age) 

Method 


Planned 

Trial  and  Error 

^ 

Chance 

Age            Number 

Number 

% 

Number 

% 

Number 

% 

5     to    6-6 
6-6  to    7-6 
7-6  to    8-6 
8-6  to    9-6 
9-6  to  10-6 
10-6  to  u-6 
11-6  to  12-6 
12-6  to  14-6 

18 
18 
23 
23 
23 
ii 
18 
20 

3 
6 

5 
13 
16 
10 
H 

12 

16 
33 

21 
56 
69 
90 

77 
60 

6 
i 
8 
6 

6 

10 

i 
6 

33 
5 
34 
26 
26 
o 
5 
30 

9 
ii 
10 

4 
I 

I 
3 

2 

50 
61 

43 
17 
4 
9 
16 

10 

This  table  shows  the  increase  of  the  use  of  the  planned 
method  until  the  age  of  10-6  where  it  is  highest,  90  per  cent. 
The  chance  method  decreases  up  to  9-6,  where  it  is  the  lowest. 

Of  thirteen  feeble-minded  delinquent  children  between  the 
ages  of  10-6  and  14-6  seen  at  the  clinic,  five  failed  to  do  the 
test  in  ten  minutes,  one  was  of  too  low  grade  mentality  to 
attempt  it,  six  did  it  by  the  chance  method  with  from  10  to 
35  repetitions  of  error,  and  one  did  it  by  the  trial  and  error 
method. 


96  CLARA  SCHMITT 

Since  the  results  show  that  the  test  is  not  suitable  for  the 
testing  of  abilities  for  its  performance  above  those  of  trial  and 
error  and  chance  for  children  under  8-6  where  82  per  cent 
use  a  method  superior  to  chance,  it  was  given  to  the  kindergarten 
children  as  a  learning  test.  After  the  first  performance  of  the 
test,  the  children  were  asked  to  do  it  again.  Those  who  had 
failed  to  accomplish  it  were  shown  how.  This  showing  con- 
sisted in  such  suggestions  as  led  the  child  to  place  the  pieces 
correctly  once.  Of  the  twenty-three  children  who  were  so 
tested,  eleven  repeated  the  test  with  no  error;  eight  with  one 
and  two  errors;  one  with  three  errors;  and  three  by  the  trial 
and  error  method.  The  last  mentioned  group  were  asked  to 
do  the  test  again.  One  of  them  did  it  with  no  error  and  two 
with  one  error  each. 

Test  IV.  Construction  Puzzle  B. — This  test  consists  of  eleven 
pieces  to  be  arranged  to  fit  six  openings.  Three  of  the  pieces 
are  of  identical  shape  and  size  and  four  others  are  paired  in 
the  same  way.  Two  of  the  openings  are  the  same  in  shape  and 
size.  Three  of  the  openings  sustain  a  one  to  one  relationship 
with  the  pieces  which  will  fill  them,  thus  leaving  three  openings 
to  be  filled  with  eight  pieces.  In  the  accomplishment  of  the 
puzzle  only  one  arrangement  of  pieces  is  possible,  with  the 
exception  of  the  two  identical  openings  which  permit  of  an 
alternate  arrangement  of  their  respective  pieces. 

In  doing  the  test  one  may  perceive  the  relationship  between 
all  the  openings  and  the  pieces  so  perfectly  as  to  accomplish 
the  task  with  no  error.  In  the  actual  performance  of  the  puzzle 
it  is  usually  accomplished  by  first  placing  the  pieces  which  have 
a  one  to  one  relationship  with  their  openings  and  thus  reducing 
the  task  to  its  simplest  form.  As  .some  of  the  pieces  when 
put  together  will  fill  some  of  the  openings  but  leave  the  task 
unaccomplished  because  there  will  be  pieces  and  openings  which 
do  not  fit,  there  is  the  possibility  of  trial  and  error  which  has 
a  show  of  possibility  of  success.  In  this  trial  and  error  the 
child  does  not  take  into  account  in  his  work  all  the  openings 
and  all  the  pieces,  but  only  the  relationship  of  part  of  the 
openings  and  part  of  the  pieces.  In  this  type  of  reaction  to 


It' 


FIG.  4 


PLATE  III 
CONSTRUCTION  TEST  B 

Another  test  for  planfulness  and  learning  by  the  method  of  trial  and 

success.     The  illustration  shows  the  test  as  presented,  and 

one  example  of  error  in  placing  the  pieces. 


From  INDIVIDUAL  DELINQUENTS — HEALY 
Courtesy  Little,  Brown  &  Co. 


STANDARDIZATION  OF  TESTS  97 

the  test  there  are  sixteen  possible  errors, — made  up  by  counting 
all  the  different  possible  ways  of  placing  all  the  pieces.  In 
addition  to  these  two  types  of  performance  there  is  another 
lower  type  of  reaction  in  which  pieces  are  placed  without  refer- 
ence to  their  spatial  relationship  to  the  openings  in  which  they 
are  placed;  as  for  example  when  a  circular  piece  is  put  into 
a  rectangular  opening.  This  type  of  reaction  if  it  does  not 
fail  to  accomplish  the  test  in  the  given  time,  does  so  by  chance. 
In  this  type  the  only  ability  measured  is  the  subject's  recog- 
nition of  success  and  his  ability  to  keep  before  him  the  object 
of  his  work  until  it  is  attained.  There  is,  of  course,  the  lower 
type  still  with  whom  the  test  would  not  be  a  possible  one,  since 
the  subject  could  not  conceive  the  object  of  the  task. 

Table  XVIII  shows  the  arrangement  of  the  data  obtained  with 
reference  to  grade. 

TABLE  XVIII 

Test  IV-     Construction   Puzzle   "B."      (By   grade) 

Errors 


17  or 
i  to  4    5  to  8    9  to  16     more   Failure 


<y 

3 

!l 

(U 

"1 
3 

1 

3 

^ 

0 

.a 
6 

o 

3 

1 

3 

Grade 

55 

< 

^ 

^ 

^ 

*R 

% 

*& 

* 

^ 

£ 

& 

E 

& 

Kdg. 

26 

3'-5i" 

2 

7 

2 

7 

7 

26 

5 

19 

2 

7 

8 

3i 

I 

20 

4'-23" 

O 

o 

5 

25 

i 

5 

4 

20 

4 

20 

6 

30 

II 

17 

2'-I'5" 

O 

o 

9 

52 

3 

17 

i 

5 

I 

5 

3 

17 

III 

21 

3-13" 

I 

4 

8 

38 

4 

19 

i 

4 

5 

23 

2 

9 

IV 

24 

2'-IO" 

6 

25 

9 

37 

5 

20 

r 

4 

2 

8 

i 

4 

V 

22 

2'-  8" 

3 

13 

6 

27 

6 

27 

3 

13 

4 

18 

o 

O 

VI 

24 

2'-I7" 

4 

16 

9 

37 

5 

2O 

4 

16 

2 

8 

0 

o 

As  was  done  in  the  preceding  tests,  the  data  of  this  one  have 
been  arranged  with  reference  to  a  qualitative  standard.  Those 
who  made  eight  errors  or  less,  that  is,  not  more  than  half  of 
the  possible  number  of  unrepeated  errors  are  classed  as  having 
done  the  test  by  the  planned  method;  those  who  made  more 
than  half  the  possible  number  of  errors  without  repeating  any, 
in  the  above  table  under  the  column  "9  to  16"  are  classed  as 
having  done  the  test  by  the  method  of  trial  and  error;  those 


98  CLARA  SCHMITT 

who  repeated  errors  or  failed  to  perform  the  test  in  ten  minutes 
are  classed  under  the  head  of  Chance.  This  is  under  the  sup- 
position that  those  who  failed  would  have  accomplished  the 
test  if  given  unlimited  time.  The  data  so  arranged  is  shown 
in  Table  XIX. 

TABLE  XIX 

Test   IV.     Construction   Puzzle   "B."      (By  grade) 

Method 


Planned  Trial  and  Error  Chance 


Grade         Number     Number          %  Number          %          Number          % 


Kdg. 

26 

II 

42 

5 

19 

10 

38 

I 

20 

6 

30 

4 

20 

10 

So 

II 

17 

12 

70 

I 

5 

4 

23 

III 

21 

13 

61 

I 

4 

7 

33 

IV 

24 

2O 

83 

I 

4 

3 

12 

V 

22 

IS 

68 

3 

13 

4 

18 

VI 

24 

18 

75 

4 

16 

2 

8 

This  table  shows  that  the  use  of  the  planned  method  was 
greatly  increased  at  the  second  grade  and  is  largest  at  the 
fourth  grade,  83  per  cent. 

Table  XX  presents  the  same  data  arranged  with  reference 
to  age. 

TABLE  XX 

Test  IV.     Construction  Puzzle  "B."     (By  age) 

Method 


Planned 

Trial  and  Error 

Chance 

Age            Number 

i 
Number 

% 

Number 

% 

'( 
Number 

% 

5     to    6-6 

18 

7 

38 

3 

16 

8 

44 

6-6  to    7-6 

18 

7 

38 

4 

22 

7 

38 

7-6  to    8-6 

23 

12 

52 

2 

8 

9 

39 

8-6  to    9-6 

23 

15 

65 

I 

4 

7 

30 

9-6  to  10-6 

23 

16 

69 

3 

13 

4 

17 

10-6  to  1  1  -6 

ii 

10 

90 

I 

9 

0 

o 

1  1-6  to  12-6 

18 

13 

3 

16 

2 

ii 

12-6  to  14-6 

20 

65 

3 

16 

4 

20 

This  table  shows  the  increase  of  the  use  of  the  planned 
method  up  to  10-6  where  it  is  highest,  90  per  cent.  The  chance 
method  decreases  up  to  this  point  where  it  is  lowest. 

Of  nineteen  feeble-minded  children  above  the  age  of  nine 
years  tried  at  the  clinic  after  the  test  came  into  use  there,  six 


STANDARDIZATION  OF  TESTS  99 

were  of  too  low  grade  to  attempt  the  test, — that  is,  they  could 
not  conceive  the  object  of  the  test  and  could  not  keep  at  work 
at  it.  Six  failed  to  do  it  in  ten  minutes,  two  did  it  by  the 
method  of  chance,  two  by  trial  and  error,  and  three  by  the 
planned  method.  Of  these  three,  one  was  13-6  years  of  age  and 
two  were  fifteen  years  of  age. 

The  test  was  given  to  the  kindergarten  children  in  two  ways. 
Those  who  failed  were  shown  how  to  do  the  puzzle.  The 
showing  consisted  of  suggestions  for  the  proper  placing  of  the 
pieces  which  the  child  carried  out.  Each  child  who  had  not 
failed  was  asked  to  do  the  puzzle  a  second  time,  and  those 
who  had  failed  were  asked  to  do  it  after  being  shown.  Of 
the  twenty-four  cases,  one  child  required  to  be  shown  a  second 
time  before  learning  to  do  the  puzzle  without  error. 

The  test  was  then  given  as  a  test  of  the  child's  ability  to 
readjust  a  learned  content  to  a  changed  situation.  The  puzzle 
board  was  turned  upsidedown  and  he  was  asked  to  do  it  again. 
In  this  situation,  the  pieces  which  were  originally  placed  at 
the  top  of  the  board  now  had  to  be  placed  at  the  bottom.  Of 
the  twenty-four  kindergarten  children  so  tested,  nineteen  made 
less  than  two  errors  in  doing  the  test  in  the  altered  position, 
and  five  made  two  errors  or  more. 

It  is  characteristic  of  the  feeble-minded  child  to  do  the  test 
under  the  altered  condition  with  the  same  amount,  or  more, 
of  trial  and  error  as  in  his  first  performance  of  the  test;  and 
sometimes  after  having  once  learned  the  test  in  one  position 
he  fails  entirely  to  do  it  in  the  other. 

The  data  for  the  above  tables  for  this  test  were  made  up 
by  counting  as  errors  the  wnong  placing  of  any  piece  and 
the  removal  of  a  rightly  placed  piece  from  its  proper  opening. 
In  some  individual  cases  this  method  of  evaluating  results  may 
be  unjust.  A  child  sometimes,  finding  that  he  can  go  no  further, 
removes  all  the  pieces  already  placed  and  begins  again,  though 
some  he  knows  are  right  and  he  replaces  them  immediately. 

The  data  are  again  rearranged  below,  made  up  by  counting 
as  errors  only  wrongly  placed  pieces  and  ignoring  the  rightly 
placed  pieces  removed  for  any  reason  from  their  proper  open- 


zoo  CLARA  SCHMITT 

ings.    According  to  this  method  the  percentaged  gradings  become 
as  follows: 

Grade  Planned      Trial  and  Error      Chance 


Kdg. 

40% 

iB% 

40% 

I 

38 

14 

47 

II 

70 

ii 

17 

III 

66 

9 

23 

IV 

87 

4 

8 

V 

77 

9 

12 

VI 

78 

20 

O 

Comparison  of  the  percentages  resulting  from  this  method 
of  reckoning  error  with  those  of  Table  XIX  shows  the  former 
to  be  slightly  more  favorable  to  a  grading  above  that  of  the 
chance  method.  Whether  this  method  affords  a  more  accurate 
judgment  of  the  mental  process  is  doubtful.  In  many  cases  the 
removal  of  a  rightly  placed  piece  is  a  positive  error,  for  instance 
that  of  the  half  circle,  since  there  is  no  other  piece  to  fill  the 
opening. 

Test  V.  Puzzle  Box. — In  the  previous  tests  the  child  had  to 
analyze  more  or  less  complicated  sets  of  spatial  relationships 
or  pattern  matching.  In  this  test  he  analyzes  a  set  of  functional 
relationships  of  a  contrivance  all  of  the  parts  of  which  are  open 
to  view,  and  involve  no  complex  mechanical  principles  such  as 
the  lever,  or  pulley,  etc.  The  test  consists  of  a  box  which  he 
is  told  he  is  to  find  a  way  of  opening.  The  necessary  number 
of  steps  to  accomplish  the  result  is  seven.  These  steps  consist 
of  the  loosening  of  the  three  inner  rings  from  their  confining 
posts,  the  removal  of  the  staple  at  the  back,  the  removal  of  the 
ring  from  the  hook  at  the  front  of  the  lock,  the  removal  of 
the  hook  itself  from  the  lock  and  the  raising  of  the  lid.  The 
arrangement  is  such  that  the  steps  must  be  accomplished  in  a 
certain  order,  and  a  tool  must  be  used  for  the  removal  of  the 
three  inner  rings.  A  long  hook  after  the  fashion  of  a  shoe 
button  hook  is  provided  for  this  purpose. 

In  giving  the  test  the  box  with  the  hook  on  top  is  placed 
lock  side  before  and  the  child  is  told  that  he  may  look  all  over 
the  box  inside  and  outside  and  any  which  way  it  occurs  to  him  to 
examine  it  to  see  if  he  can  find  a  way  to  open  it,  and  that  he  may 
do  anything  he  thinks  will  help  in  opening  it  or  use  anything  he 


toO 


FIG.  5 


PLATE  IV 

A  PUZZLE  Box — OUR  TEST  V 

An  example  of  a  concrete  problem  to  be  reasoned  out  from  perceived 

relationships.     Each  step  to  the  solution,  namely,  opening 

the  box,  is  plainly  visible. 


From  INDIVIDUAL  DELINQUENTS — HEALY 
Courtesy  Little,  Brown  &  Co. 


STANDARDIZATION  OF  TESTS  101 

thinks  will  help.  No  further  hint  concerning  the  tool  is  given 
him,  except  in  case  he  tries  to  accomplish  step  one,  the  first 
one  it  is  necessary  to  manipulate,  without  the  tool  and  leaves 
it  to  try  something  else  since  he  can  not  succeed.  He  is  then 
told  that  he  may  use  the  hook.  He  is  then  recorded  in  the 
classification  under  tool  idea — .  The  child  who  is  not  classified 
under  tool  idea — sees  for  himself  the  need  of  the  tool  and  uses 
it  without  suggestion  or  asks  permission  to  do  so.  In  the 
record  of  data  the  time  which  the  child  spends  in  studying  the 
box  without  touching  any  of  the  fastenings  is  recorded,  each 
step  or  attempt,  and  the  time  to  accomplish  the  test.  Wrong 
attempts  are  recorded  as  errors.  The  data  for  the  time  spent 
studying  the  box  before  proceeding  to  work  were  found  to 
have  no  correlation  with  anything  else.  Whether  the  child  spent 
a  few  seconds  or  several  minutes  in  such  study  had  no  relation 
to  his  age  or  to  the  quality  of  his  performance  in  doing  the 
test  after  he  began.  Table  XXI  shows  the  data  obtained  ar- 
ranged with  reference  to  grade.  The  qualitative  classification 
was  made  as  follows :  a  child  was  placed  in  the  trial  and  error 
column  if  after  his  manipulation  of  step  one  he  made  any 
other  errors  before  accomplishing  the  opening  of  the  box.  He 
was  placed  in  the  planned  column  if,  after  the  manipulation  of 
step  one,  he  made  no  further  errors  in  opening  the  box.  The 
one  exception  to  this  was  the  attempt  to  do  step  five,  removing 
the  ring  from  the  hook  at  the  lock,  after  step  three.  This  error 
is  permitted  for  the  reason  that  after  step  three  has  been  done 
the  string  holding  the  ring  of  step  five  is  somewhat  loosened 
and  one  can  only  know  by  trying  it  whether  it  is  sufficiently 
loose  to  permit  of  the  removal  of  the  ring  from  the  hook.  It 
may  also  be  explained  here  that  error  six  is  an  attempt  to 
push  the  hook  through  the  lock  with  one  movement  instead  of 
making  the  turn  in  the  lock  which  is  necessary  to  permit  of  its 
removal.  In  this  classification  the  assumption  is  made  that  in 
the  attempts  preliminary  to  step  one,  (and  no  case  has  been 
seen  in  which  some  were  not  made),  the  child  does  or  does 
not  learn  the  arrangement  of  the  fastenings  and  their  rela- 
tionship to  each  other.  If  his  learning  has  been  complete  he 


102  CLARA  SCHMITT 

can  then  proceed  without  further  error.  If  it  has  not  been 
complete  other  errors  are  made,  and  he  then  can  open 
the  box  only  by  a  trial  and  error  process.  The  usual  procedure 
on  the  part  of  the  child  in  this  learning  process  is  to  take  up 
the  box  and  trace  the  fastenings  back  from  the  lock.  He  ex- 
amines the  lock  and  the  ring  of  step  five,  follows  it  back  to 
step  four,  and  so  on  back  to  step  one.  He  usually  tries  one 
or  all  of  them  before  arriving  at  step  one  and  sometimes  does 
not  follow  the  series  through  the  ,first  time  but  goes  back  to 
the  lock  or  some  other  point  and  tries  some  of  the  fastenings 
again. 

TABLE  XXI 
Test  V.     Puzzle  Box.     (By  grade) 

i Method \      Average 

Trial  and  Tool    number          Average 

Failed      error    Planned      idea     moves  time 


S 

1 

<u 
| 

<u 

S 

3 

<L> 

.0 

3 

1 

c 

e 
.5 

T3 

rt 

w 

<U 

JS 

rt£ 
£  t 

Grade 

fc 

* 

*fc 

*£. 

1 

& 

PH 

H 

S 

H 

*I 

21 

12 

57 

9  loo 

O 

0 

2 

9 

O 

16 

7'-23" 

II 

17 

2 

ii 

14    94 

I 

5 

5 

29 

HI 

14 

6'-52" 

7'-56" 

III 

21 

3 

12 

16    90 

2 

9 

9 

42 

9 

15 

3'-  5" 

6'-  2" 

IV 

24 

2 

8 

15    70 

7 

29 

12 

50 

9 

14 

5'-33" 

6'-i6" 

V 

22 

O 

o 

8    36 

14 

63 

3 

13 

9 

13 

2'-52" 

4'-45" 

VI 

23 

O 

o 

7    30 

16 

69 

o 

o 

9 

12 

2'-52" 

3'-54" 

*  A  box   similar   to   the   one   used  there   was   tried   with   ten   first   grade 
children  in  another  school.    All  of  them  failed  to  open  it  in  10  minutes. 

Table  XXI  shows  that  for  the  children  under  the  fifth  grade 
from  70  to  100  per  cent  are  able  to  do  the  test  by  a  method 
not  superior  to  that  of  trial  and  error.  That  the  number  of 
those  who  lack  the  tool  idea  in  their  planning  of  the  test  in- 
creases up  to  the  fifth  grade  is  due  to  the  fact  that  it  is  taken 
account  of  only  for  those  who  succeeded  in  doing  the  test.  In 
the  fourth  grade  where  the  amount  of  failure  is  the  smallest 
the  lack  of  tool  idea  is  greatest.  The  lack  of  the  tool  idea  is 
generally  associated  with  the  trial  and  error  method  of  doing 
the  test;  of  the  31  children  who  lacked  the  tool  idea,  28  did 
the  test  by  the  trial  and  error  method;  the  three  who  did  it  by 


STANDARDIZATION  OF  TESTS  103 

the  planned  method  belonged  to  the  fourth  and  fifth  grades. 
The  last  two  columns  of  the  table  show  the  time  correlation 
with  the  two  methods  of  doing  the  test.  The  time  decreases 
slightly  as  the  grade  progresses  and  for  each  grade  the  time 
for  doing  the  test  by  the  planned  method  averages  less  than  the 
time  for  the  trial  and  error  method.  This  time  correlation 
with  grade  is  probably  due  to  increase  of  motor  ability.  The 
two  columns  of  the  table  preceding  the  last  two  show  the 
correlation  of  number  of  moves  with  the  two  methods  of  work. 
The  number  oi  moves  in  each  case  is  the  number  of  errors 
plus  seven,  the  number  of  correct  moves  necessary.  After  the 
second  grade  the  average  number  of  preliminary  errors,  found 
in  subtracting  seven  from  the  average  number  of  moves  in 
each  column,  is  two,  and  does  not  decrease.  The  number  of 
moves  decreases  little  for  those  who  did  the  test  by  trial  and 
error. 

Table  XXII  shows  the  same  data  arranged  with  reference 
to  age. 

TABLE  XXII 

Test  V.     Puzzle  Box.     (By  age) 

Method 


Average 

Trial  and  number      Average 

Failed       error     Planned  moves  time 


g 

3 

1 

1 

1 

•0 

rt  «i 

*c3  £ 

•c 
<u 

•o 

•d 

<u 

c 
JS 

Age 

fe 

525 

*R 

55 

* 

55 

*R 

H 

Pk 

h 

E 

6-6  to    7-6 

8 

4 

50 

4 

IOO 

o 

o 

17 

7'-39" 

7-6  to    8-6 

27 

7 

26 

20 

100 

0 

o 

14 

7'-46" 

8-6  to    9-6 

23 

3 

12 

17 

87 

3 

12 

14 

9 

6-55 

4  -21 

9-6  to  10-6 

23 

2 

8 

15 

73 

6 

26 

14 

10 

5-33 

4'-43" 

10-6  to  1  1-6 

ii 

0 

o 

3 

27 

8 

72 

13 

9 

5-  3' 

3'-43" 

1  1  -6  to  12-6 

,18 

0 

o 

6 

33 

12 

66 

13 

9 

4'-3o" 

2'-38" 

12-6  to  14-6 

19 

o 

o 

8 

42 

II 

'57 

10 

9 

3-25 

2-55' 

This  table  shows  that  the  use  of  the  planned  method  for 
this  test  increases  greatly  at  the  age  of  10-6  where  it  is  the 
highest,  72  per  cent. 


104  CLARA  SCHMITT 

Of  twenty-six  children  between  the  ages  of  106  and  17-6, 
judged  at  the  clinic  to  be  feeble-minded,  and  for  whom  a 
record  for  this  test  was  recorded,  eleven  were  of  too  low  grade 
to  attempt  the  test,  ten  failed  in  fifteen  minutes  to  accomplish 
it  and  five  did  it  by  trial  and  error. 

Test  IX.  Cross  Line  A. — This  and  the  two  following  tests 
constitute  a  series  of  increasing  difficulty,  with  the  climax  at 
the  reasoning  step  of  the  third  of  the  series,  the  code  test.  In 
this  test  the  child  uses  representative  material  to  accomplish  the 
desired  result.  In  the  previous  tests  the  child's  work  is  con- 
tinually checked  up  by  the  sense  stimulus  of  the  concrete  material 
with  which  he  works.  This  and  the  two  following  tests  test 
his  ability  to  analyse  his  memory  of  the  figure  which  has  just 
been  drawn  before  him. 


AV  >  < 

The  cross  lines  represented  in  the  above  figure  are  drawn 
before  the  child  and  he  is  told  that  in  the  space  with  the  lines 
going  upward  and  opening  upward  an  I  is  placed;  in  the  space 
opening  out  to  one  side  a  2  is  placed;  in  the  space  with  its 
lines  going  downward  a  3  is  placed,  and  in  the  space  opening 
out  to  the  other  side  a  4  is  placed.  While  he  is  being  told 
this  each  space  is  outlined  with  the  pencil  and  the  number 
is  written  in.  Then  one  of  the  elements  of  the  figure  is  drawn 
at  one  side,  and  he  is  asked  to  tell  which  one  of  the  spaces 
it  is  like.  If  he  answers  correctly,  his  reason  for  his  answer 


STANDARDIZATION  OF  TESTS  105 

is  asked.  If  he  answers  to  the  effect  that  its  lines  point  like 
or  open  up  like  the  one  he  has  named,  one  may  assume  that 
he  understands  the  problem,  and  go  on  with  the  next  step  in 
the  test.  If  he  answers  wrongly,  the  figure  is  discussed  further 
with  him  until  he  understands  the  nature  of  the  analysis  to 
be  made. 

There  are  cases  of  such  low  mentality  as  to  be  unable  to 
comprehend  the  problem.  When  the  child's  comprehension  of 
the  problem  is  sure,  the  figure  which  has  been  drawn  for  him 
is  covered,  with  the  remark  that  he  may  now  see  if  he  can 
do  the  same  thing  with  it  covered  up. 

The  elements  are  then  drawn  for  him  one  by  one  and  he  is 
asked  to  number  them.  The  question  asked  him  is,  which  space 
is  this  one  like?  He  is  provided  with  a  pencil  to  place  the 
appropriate  number.  If  he  begins  by  making  errors  he  can 
sometimes  be  led  to  find  a  method  of  recall  .for  himself  by 
the  suggestion  that  he  think  of  the  covered  up  figure  and  see 
if  he  can  remember  what  kind  of  space  one  is  in,  and  two,  and 
three  and  so  on.  If,  after  the  four  spaces  are  drawn  and  num- 
bered there  are  errors,  he  is  asked  to  draw  the  figure  himself. 
In  case  of  error  one  wishes  to  know  if  he  has  remembered  the 
figure  and  its  scheme  of  numbering  incorrectly,  but  has  analyzed 
correctly  according  to  his  memory  of  it,  or  if  he  has  remembered 
it  correctly  and  analyzed  it  incorrectly.  If  he  has  analyzed  it 
correctly  as  he  has  remembered  it  he  is  placed  in  the  list  of 
those  who  have  succeeded  at  the  first  attempt,  since  it  is  his 
ability  to  do  the  analysis  correctly  that  is  to  be  tested.  If 
he  has  not  analyzed  correctly  but  has  remembered  correctly,  he 
is  told  that  he  did  not  number  the  spaces  correctly  at  first  and 
that  he  may  try  again.  If  he  has  neither  analyzed  nor  re- 
membered correctly  he  is  permitted  to  look  at  the  original 
figure  and  then  is  asked  to  draw  it  again,  and  is  given  as  many 
trials  as  is  necessary  to  learn  to  draw  the  figure  and  number 
it  correctly  from  memory.  The  writer  has  found  no  child  who 
has  been  able  to  comprehend  the  problem  of  the  test  who  could 
not  learn  to  draw  and  number  the  figure  correctly  with  as 
much  as  three  such  trials.  If,  after  the  second  attempt  at 


io6 


CLARA  SCHMITT 


analysis  from  memory,  he  still  .fails  to  number  all  the  elements 
correctly  he  is  again  asked  to  draw  the  figure  and  the  process 
is  repeated  as  before.  He  is  given  four  such  trials  at  the  analysis 
before  being  classed  as  failure.  Table  XXIII  shows  the  data 
obtained  with  this  test  arranged  with  reference  to  grade.  It 
cannot  be  given  to  kindergarten  children  because  of  their 
unfamiliarity  with  written  numerals. 

TABLE  XXIII 

Test  IX.     Cross  Line  Test  A.     (By  Grade) 
Failure  Succeeded 


Fourth 

trial 


First 
trial 


Second 
trial 


Third 
trial 


Fourth 
trial 


1 

1 

3 

1 

3 

1 

3 

i 

Grade 

fc 

55 

* 

^ 

* 

5 

5 

fc 

£ 

* 

& 

I 

20 

2 

10 

12 

00 

3 

15 

3 

IS 

o 

o 

II 

17 

2 

II 

14 

82 

i 

5 

o 

0 

o 

o 

III 

21 

0 

o 

19 

90 

n 

4 

i 

4 

o 

o 

IV 

25 

0 

o 

21 

84 

i 

4 

ii 

4 

2 

8 

V 

22 

O 

0 

22 

IOO 

o 

O 

0 

o 

O 

0 

VI 

24 

O 

o 

24 

IOO 

o 

0 

o 

o 

o 

0 

The  table  shows  that  the  percentage  of  children  between  the 
first  and  the  sixth  grades  who  fail  to  do  the  test  is  negligible, 
and  that  after  the  first  grade  the  percentage  who  need  more 


FIG.  7 


12. 


I 

i 

(c. 

7 

1. 

$• 

8 

3 

(> 

7 

o  L  m  j 

r  u  D  n 


STANDARDIZATION  OF  TESTS  107 

than  a  first  trial  is  negligible.  Since  the  results  .for  the  grades 
are  so  uniform  an  age  table  is  omitted. 

Test  X,  Cro'ss  Line  Test  B. — The  procedure  for  this  test 
is,  so  far  as  its  own  circumstances  permit,  like  that  of  the 
preceding  test.  The  figure  is  constructed  before  the  child,  and 
the  spaces  in  which  i,  2,  and  3  are  placed  are  outlined  while 
being  numbered.  He  is  then  given  .four  trials,  proceeding  as 
prescribed  in  the  preceding  test.  The  results  are  evaluated  as 
in  the  preceding  test. 

Table  XXIV  shows  the  data  of  this  test  arranged  with  refer- 
ence to  grade. 

TABLE  XXIV 
Test  X.    Cross  Line  Test  B 
Failure  Succeeded 


Fourth  First  Second  Third  Fourth 

trial  trial  trial  trial  trial 


1 

3 

1 

3 

1 

1 

| 

1 

Grade 

Jz; 

£ 

*• 

£ 

* 

55 

& 

fc 

* 

£ 

& 

I 

20 

6 

30 

8 

40 

3 

15 

2 

10 

I 

S 

II 

17 

2 

III 

ii 

64 

I 

5 

2 

II 

I 

5 

III 

21 

I 

4 

17 

80 

3 

14 

O 

o 

o 

0 

IV 

25 

O 

o 

20 

80 

I 

4 

2 

8 

2 

8 

V 

22 

0 

o 

18 

81 

3 

13 

I 

4 

0 

o 

VI 

24 

o 

o 

24 

IOO 

o 

0 

0 

o 

O 

o 

This  table  shows  that  after  the  first  grade  the  percentage 
of  failure  is  negligible,  and  that  after  the  third  grade  there  is 
no  failure  in  doing  the  test.  After  the  second  grade  from  80 
per  cent  to  100  per  cent  are  able  to  do  the  test  at  the  first  trial. 
Before  the  second  grade  the  percentage  of  children  who  need 
more  than  a  second  trial  is  negligible. 

The  writer's  further  experience  with  this  and  the  preceding 
cross  line  test  has  led  to  the  opinion  that  two  trials  constitute 
a  sufficient  test  of  the  child's  ability  to  perform  this  type  of 
mental  process.  The  drawing  of  the  figure  by  the  child  after 
the  first  unsuccessful  attempt  shows  whether  the  failure  is  due 
to  his  having  forgotten  the  figure  and  its  number  arrangement. 


io8  CLARA  SCHMITT 

It  gives  him  in  an  added  experience,  the  kinaesthetic,  whatever 
it  may  be  worth  in  controlling  his  imagery  for  the  second  trial. 
The  practice  effect  which  more  repetition  may  have  upon  the 
result  is  eliminated  with  the  .fewer  trials. 

Test  XL  Code  Test. — To  this  test  has  been  added  a  step 
which  was  not  involved  in  the  test  as  described  by  Healy  and 
Fernald.  This  added  step  requires  the  subject  to  use  his  ex- 
perience with  the  material  of  the  two  preceding  tests  in  a  new 
way.  It  is  a  test  in  reasoning  which  controls  the  material 
which  the  child  uses  for  the  new  product  and  which  is  not 
the  result  of  previous  learning  as  is  the  case  with  many  other 
reasoning  tests  in  use. 

Bonsel  (21 )  used  in  testing  the  reasoning  ability  of  children  (i ) 
arithmetical  problems :  //  three-quarters  of  a  gallon  of  oil  costs 
9  cents  what  will  7  gallons  cost?  What  number  subtracted  12 
times  from  jo  will  leave  a  remainder  of  6?  (2)  The  com- 
pleting of  sentences  to  agree  with  the  fact  implied  in  the  sen- 
tence : — always  comes  in  the  last  week  of  December.  The  flesh 
of  cattle  used  for  food  is  called — .  (3)  The  selection  of  alter- 
nate statements  to  agree  with  the  fact  implied  in  the  statement : 

Days  are     .   *       in  summer  than  in  winter.     Men  are  usually 
shorter 

,  than  women.  (4)  Opposites:  day,  asleep.  (5)  Selec- 
tive judgment,  in  which  a  number  of  reasons  are  given  to  show 
why  New  York  has  become  a  greater  city  than  Boston;  why 
oak  is  superior  to  pine  for  the  making  of  furniture,  etc.,  and 
the  child  selects  those  which  are  in  his  judgment  the  most 
adequate.  (6)  Literary  interpretations :  This  little  rill,  that  from 
the  springs  of  yonder  grove  its  current  brings,  etc.  All  of  these 
tests  involve  previous  instruction,  and  it  is  possible  that  they 
may  test  nothing  more  than  the  thoroughness  of  the  child's 
assimilation  of  such  instruction.  It  is  also  possible  and  even 
probable  that  one's  ability  to  learn  in  this  way  the  reasoning 
processes  taught  by  others  is  positively  correlated  with  his  own 
ability  to  reason,  and  thus  a  measure  of  the  former  becomes 
an  indirect  measure  of  the  latter.  None  of  the  Bonser  tests, 


STANDARDIZATION  OF  TESTS 


109 


however,  control  the  material  or  the  process  which  is  used  in 
the  act  of  reasoning.  In  doing  the  tests  the  child  may  never 
have  reasoned  at  all,  but  have  relied  only  upon  information 
previously  acquired  and  used.  Nothing  else  could  be  the  case 
with  tests  two,  three,  four  and  five. 

The  procedure  for  Test  XI  must  necessarily  be  adapted  to 
the  interests  and  temperament  of  the  child  to  whom  it  is  given. 
Only  general  directions  for  giving  it  can  be  suggested.  The 
procedure  adopted  by  the  writer  is  as  follows : 

One  says  to  the  child,  "You  know  that  in  war  time,  when 
two  armies  are  fighting  each  other,  the  generals  send  out  spies 
to  find  out  the  secrets  of  the  enemy's  army,  and  sometimes 
the  spy  must  write  what  he  has  found  in  a  secret  message  to 
his  general.  When  he  writes  a  secret  message,  of  course,  he 
cannot  write  it  in  the  way  in  which  we  ordinarily  write  a  letter 
because  it  might  fall  into  the  hands  of  the  enemy  and  betray 
him.  He  must  have  a  secret  way  of  writing  so  that  only  the 
general  and  himself  will  be  able  to  read  the  message.  Now, 
I  will  show  you  something  from  which  we  can  get  a  kind  of 
secret  writing.  The  code  is  here  constructed  before  him  while 
he  gives  attention. 

FIG.  8 


a 

d 

9- 

(r 

& 

A 

c 

T 

/C 

j. 

m 

* 

A 

<* 

rv 

^- 

{* 

•• 

y- 

~7F)  LJQ 


no  CLARA  SCHMITT 

When  the  four  figures  are  constructed  in  which  the  letters 
of  the  alphabet  must  be  placed,  the  child  is  asked  to  repeat 
the  alphabet  while  they  are  being  put  in  the  spaces.  This  insures 
his  attention  to  the  rather  long  process  in  hand,  and  also  shows 
whether  he  knows  the  alphabet  serially  and  hence  might  be 
expected  to  do  the  second  part  of  the  test.  Some  children  do 
not  know  the  alphabet  in  its  correct  serial  order. 

After  the  construction  has  been  completed,  then  he  is  asked, 
"Now,  what  could  one  get  from  this  to  use  instead  of  the 
writing  letters  of  the  alphabet  for  his  message?  If,  for  in- 
stance, he  were  going  to  write  a  word  that  began  with  A, 
what  could  he  use  instead  of  A,  and  instead  of  B,  or  C  or 
any  other  letter  he  might  need  to  use  in  writing  his  message?" 

The  children  frequently  make  suggestions  which  have  no 
relation  to  the  construction  before  them.  They  will  say  he 
may  use  numbers  instead  of  letters,  or  they  offer  the  Morse 
code,  or  some  similar  scheme  with  which  they  are  already 
familiar.  After  each  wrong  suggestion  it  is  pointed  out  to  them 
that  their  scheme  has  nothing  to  do  with  the  one  before  them, 
and  could  not  be  derived  from  it,  which  they,  of  course,  readily 
admit.  The  writer  permits  two  such  suggestions,  giving  them 
an  opportunity  to  make  a  third  before  classing  the  child  among 
the  failures  for  the  reasoning  step  of  the  test.  Those  who 
succeed  in  seeing  the  correct  process  for  the  code  writing  are 
classed  in  the  column,  "Idea  -f-"  in  Table  XXV  below.  Those 
who  fail  through  having  made  only  wrong  suggestions,  or  hav- 
ing made  none  at  all,  are  placed  in  the  column,  "Idea — ."  Those 
who  have  no  suggestions  to  make  are  allowed  to  think  about  it, 
and  urged  to  think  about  it  until  they  themselves  declare  that 
they  can  see  no  way  of  getting  the  secret  alphabet  from  the 
scheme  before  them.  Those  who  fail  are  then  shown  that  each 
letter  may  be  represented  by  the  space  in  which  it  stands, 
and  are  asked  to  write  the  symbol  which  would  stand  for  G, 
the  one  which  would  stand  for  P,  the  one  which  would  stand 
for  V,  and  the  one  for  Z  in  order  to  bring  out  the  idea  of 
the  relation  of  the  dot  to  the  scheme,  and  also  for  the  purpose 
of  giving  a  slight  practice.  Those  who  have  succeeded  are 
also  given  this  same  practice. 


STANDARDIZATION  OF  TESTS  m 

The  figure  is  then  covered,  and  the  child  is  asked  to  write 
the  message,  "Come  quickly,"  previously  written  on  the  page 
before  him.  He  is  told  to  take  as  much  time  as  is  necessary 
to  enable  him  to  write  it  without  errors. 

Table  XXV  shows  the  data  obtained  from  this  test  arranged 
according  to  grade. 

TABLE  XXV 
Test  XI.     Code  Test 


Grade 

Number 

Idea 

,  '  — 
Number 

N 

Idea 

,  "— 
Number 

—  \ 

Idea? 
'Number 

Average 
number 
errors 

II 
III 
IV 
V 
VI 

16 

21 

25 
22 
24 

3 

10 

15 
I'S 

12 

18 

44 
60 
68 
50 

13 
9 
9 
6 

10 

81 
42 
36 
26 
40 

2 
I 
I 
2 

8 

7 
4 
2 

3 

This  table  shows  that  after  the  third  grade  more  than  50 
per  cent  of  the  children  succeeded  in  doing  the  reasoning  step 
of  the  test.  In  the  column  "Idea?"  was  placed  those  children 
who  had  seen  this  code  previously.  After  the  third  grade 
they  are  able  to  attend  to  this  rather  long  and  complex  process 
of  analysis  of  mental  imagery  to  such  an  extent  as  to  average 
not  more  than  four  errors  or  36  per  cent  out  of  the  possible 
eleven. 

The  second  grade  showed  itself  almost  wholly  unable  to  do 
the  reasoning  step  of  the  test,  and  was  rather  uninterested  in 
the  test  itself.  The  necessity  for  a  secret  means  of  communica- 
tion is  quite  outside  the  range  of  experience  of  these  children, 
since  for  them  writing  itself  has  not  yet  wholly  ceased  to  be 
a  mysterious  process. 

Of  thirty-two  children  between  the  ages  of  8-6  and  17-6, 
classified  as  feeble-minded  at  the  clinic,  twelve  were  of  too 
low  grade  mentally  to  undertake  any  of  the  three  above  de- 
scribed tests.  Twelve  failed  in  Tests  IX  and  X.  With  Test 
IX,  four  succeeded  at  the  first  trial,  two  succeeded  at  the  second 
trial,  one  succeeded  at  the  third  trial  and  one  succeeded  at  the 
fourth  trial.  Among  those  who  succeeded  with  Test  IX  two 
succeeded  at  the  second  trial  with  Test  X. 

The  Code  Test  was  not  attempted  with  any  of  these  children 


112  CLARA  SCHMITT 

because  of  their  difficulty  with  the  two  tests  which  necessarily 
precede  it. 

Test  XV.   Association  of  verbal  opposites. — The  list  of  words 
used  for  this  test  is  as  follows : 

Good  Big  Happy  Sick  Empty 

Outside  Loud  Cheap  Glad  Many 

Quick  Black  Dead  Thin  Above 

Tall  Light  Rich  War  Friend 

The  child  is  given  some  trial  practice  with  as  many  words 
outside  of  this  list  as  is  necessary  to  show  that  he  has  gained 
the  correct  idea  of  what  is  desired  of '  him  in  this  test.  He 
is  asked  to  say  the  word  which  means  just  the  opposite  of 
the  word  which  will  be  pronounced  to  him,  as  quickly  as  he 
can  think  of  it.  The  time  is  recorded  with  a  stop  watch.  With 
this  test,  as  with  many  others,  the  absolute  time  of  response  is, 
within  rather  large  limits,  of  less  importance  than  the  character 
of  the  response.  The  data  of  the  test  comprises  three  things : 
The  reaction  time,  the  errors  made  in  response,  and  the  fail- 
ures. An  error  is  a  reaction  which  is  not  in  idea  an  antonym 
of  the  stimulus  word.  Among  defective  individuals  it  is  very 
common  to  find  a  lack  of  control  of  the  associations  which 
may  be  aroused  by  any  stimulus  word.  The  normal  individual 
will  repress  the  wrong  associations  and  give  only  the  one  which 
is  desired.  The  defective  individual  instead  of  giving  an 
antonym  will  give  a  synonym,  or  anything  else  which  may  come 
immediately  to  his  mind  as  an  association  with  the  stimulus 
word.  The  defective  child  often,  too,  will  embody  his  reaction 
word,  whether  correct  or  not,  in  an  entire  sentence.  The  nor- 
mal child  inhibits  all  words  but  the  one  which  is  desired.  In 
recording  the  data  any  such  response  which  was  not  in  its 
meaning  an  antonym  of  the  stimulus  word,  was  recorded  as 
error.  Care  was  taken,  however,  in  each  case,  if  the  reaction 
word  was  not  of  the  same  part  of  speech  as  the  stimulus  word 
or,  if  it  were  not  the  classical  antonym  of  the  stimulus  word, 
to  determine  whether  in  the  child's  mind  it  was  an  antonym. 
This  is  especially  desirable  in  testing  children  from  homes  where 
a  foreign  language  is  spoken,  or  from  parochial  schools.  For 


STANDARDIZATION  OF  TESTS  113 

instance,  the  classical  antonym  for  empty  is  full;  children  fre- 
quently respond  with  the  word  filled;  the  classical  antonym  for 
above  is  below;  a  frequent  response  is  down;  the  classical 
antonym  for  sick  is  well;  a  frequent  response  from  children  is 
better.  Slang  words  are  always  recorded  as  correct.  For  in- 
stance, good  gets  the  frequent  response  bum.  One  might  go 
on  to  enumerate  many  other  instances  in  which  the  child's 
mental  action  was  controlled  in  the  desired  way,  but  in  which 
response  was  modified  by  educational  experience. 

A  failure  is  a  lack  of  response  of  any  sort  within  ten  seconds 
after  the  pronunciation  of  the  stimulus  word.  With  failure 
it  is  desirable  to  know  what  is  the  reason  for  the  failure, — 
whether  it  is  lack  of  knowledge,  or  whether  the  child  could 
not  bring  about  the  right  association.  To  determine  this,  ques- 
tions may  be  asked.  For  instance,  if  he  fails  to  respond  at 
all  to  the  word  loud,  one  may  ask  him,  "If  the  noise  is  not  loud 
what  kind  of  a  noise  is  it?"  and  the  question  may  be  put  in 
various  ways  to  bring  about  in  his  own  mind  the  correct  asso- 
ciation. If  in  this  way  one  elicits  the  correct  response,  then 
he  is  recorded  as  failure  because  of  slow  response.  If  the 
correct  response  cannot  be  elicited,  he  is  then  recorded  as  failure 
from  lack  of  knowledge. 

Table  XXVI  shows  the  data  obtained  from  this  test  arranged 
according  to  grade.  The  failures  were  recorded  as  such  only 
if  they  were  failures  because  of  slow  response,  and  not  because 
of  lack  of  knowledge.  The  most  common  failures  for  lack  of 
knowledge  were  to  the  stimulus  words  war  and  friend. 
Younger  children  have  not  had  sufficient  experience  in  reading 
or  conversation  to  know  the  opposite  of  war.  The  response  to 
the  stimulus  word  friend  is  frequently  a  word  indicating  a  rela- 
tive or  member  of  the  family,  showing  that  in  the  child's  mind 
there  are  two  classes  of  persons  with  whom  he  has  personal 
relationship,  those  belonging  to  the  family  and  those  outside 
of  the  family  who  are  friends. 


ii4 


CLARA  SCHMITT 

TABLE  XXVI 
Test  XV.    Opposites  Test.     (By  giade) 

Errors  Failures 


5  or 
o  i  &  2     3  &  4      more 


I  &  2      3  &  4 


5  or 
more 


V 

a 

J8 

E 
:3 

fcJ 
>  •*-* 

"1 

3 

J2 

S 

£ 
P 

,0 

P 

.a 

| 

.0 

P 

,Q 

| 

O 

< 

*& 

% 

*R 

5? 

*R 

£ 

*R 

*R 

^ 

*R 

*Z*        fe^ 

^    ^ 

I 

20 

23^ 

2 

IO 

12 

60 

4 

25 

! 

5 

3 

IS 

IO 

50 

6    30 

i      5 

II 

16 

2H 

8 

50 

6 

37 

I' 

6 

Ii 

6 

6 

37 

6 

37 

3     18 

i      6 

III 

21 

2^/5 

6 

28 

12 

57 

3 

14 

0 

o 

3 

14 

ii 

52 

4     19 

3     14 

IV 

23 

2 

14 

61 

8 

34 

i 

4 

O 

o 

ii 

47 

9 

39 

2        8 

i      4 

V 

22 

't-Ys 

17 

77 

4 

17 

i 

4 

O 

o 

15 

67 

7 

31 

O        0 

o     o 

VI 

24 

13/S 

211 

87 

3 

12 

o 

o 

o 

o 

13 

54 

IO 

41 

i      4 

o     o 

The  table  shows  that,  beginning  with  the  second  grade,  the 
percentage  of  children  who  make  more  than  two  errors  become 
negligible;  beginning  with  the  fourth  grade,  from  61  to  87 
per  cent  make  no  errors.  On  the  failure  side  we  see  that, 
beginning  with  the  fourth  grade  the  percentage  of  those  who 
make  more  than  two  failures  becomes  negligible,  and  at  the 
fifth  grade  more  than  fifty  per  cent  of  the  children  make  no 
failures. 

Table  XXVII  shows  the  same  data  arranged  with  reference 
to  age.  The  same  statement  with  reference  to  errors  as  above 


TABLE  XXVII 

Test  XV.     Opposite  Test.     (By  age) 
Errors 


5  or 
i  &  2     3  &  4      more 


Failures 


5  or 
I  &  2      3  &  4      more 


Age 

I-,   <U  "> 

0    CC   - 

^3  «  w 

S  fc  E 
3  >'J 

5?  < 

Number 

* 

Numiber 

& 

Numiber 

* 

1 
I 

& 

i 

& 

Numlber 

* 

Numiber 

* 

1 

* 

6-6  to 
7-6  to 
8-6  to 
9-6  to 
10-6  to 
n-6  to 
12-6  to 

7-6 
8-6 
9-6 
10-6 
11-6 

12-6 

14^6 

8  2^ 
24  2J3 
23  2 
23   1^ 
II   I3/^ 

17   1^ 
20   1'^ 

o 
9 
9 
15 
6 

13 

17 

0 

37 
39 
65 
54 
76 
85 

5 
ii 
ii 
8 
4 
3 
3 

62 

45 
47 
34 
36 
17 
13 

3 

2 

3 
o 
I 
I 
o 

37 
8 

12 
O 

9 

5 
o 

o 

2 
0 
O 

o 

0 

o 

o 
8 
o 
o 
o 
o 
o 

o 

6 
9 
9 

4 

IB 
II 

O 

25 

39 
39 
36 
61 
55 

6 
ii 

9 

ii 
5 
6 
8 

75 
45 
39 
47 
45 
35 
40 

i 

6 
3 

2 
I 
O 

I 

12 

25 

8 

9 
o 

5 

I 
i 

2 
I 
I 
O 
O 

12 
4 
8 
4 
9 
o 
o 

STANDARDIZATION  OF  TESTS  115 

may  be  deduced  from  this  table  beginning  with  the  age  8-6, 
and  for  failures  beginning  with  the  age  9-6.  At  n-6  more 
than  76  per  cent  of  the  children  make  no  errors  and  61  per 
cent  no  failures. 

Of  thirty  feeble-minded  children  between  the  ages  of  9-6 
and  17-6  seen  at  the  clinic,  twenty-one  were  unable  to  under- 
stand the  import  of  the  test  and  so  control  their  associations 
as  to  make  a  record  for  it;  the  defective  child  fails  to  inhibit 
the  wrong  association,  responds  with  a  whole  sentence,  goes  off 
on  a  tangent  of  discussion  with  each  stimulus  word,  or  re- 
sponds with  absolute  silence.  Of  the  nine  remaining,  four 
made  two  errors  or  less,  two  made  either  three  and  four  errors, 
and  three  made  five  or  more  errors.  Five  made  two  failures 
or  less,  one  made  three  failures,  and  three  made  five  or  more 
failures. 

Texi  XII,  Memory  from  Visual  Verbal  Presentation. — A 
typewritten  sheet  like  the  ordinary  printed  page,  containing  the 
following  selection  is  placed  before  the  child : 

If  a  man  finds  that  the  house  is  on  fire,  he  should  first  look  to  see  if  it  is 
a  large  fire.  If  it  is  a  small  one,  he  should  quickly  pour  water  on  it  or  smother 
it.  But  if  it  is  large,  he  should  run  to  the  fire  alarm  box,  calling  out  fire 
to  the  other  people  in  the  house.  Then  he  should  go  back  and  help  old  or 
sick  people  or  little  children  to  escape  from  the  burning  building.  When  all 
the  people  are  out,  if  there  is  time  he  may  save  valuable  things  such  as  money 
or  jewelry.  Then  when  the  fire  engine  comes,  he  may  keep  the  crowds  or 
curious  people  out  of  the  way  so  that  the  firemen  may  work  more  easily. 

The  child  is  told  that  he  may  read  this  selection  to  himself 
once  and  then  hand  it  to  the  experimenter  and  tell  what  he  has 
read ;  just  as  nearly  like  that  which  he  read  as  he  can  remember ; 
but  that  if  he  can  not  remember  it  precisely  he  should  not  be 
worried  about  it  but  give  it  as  best  he  can.  The  request  to 
hand  it  back  as  soon  as  he  has  finished  reading,  is  to  let  the 
experimenter  know  that  he  has  finished,  and  to  discourage  his 
attempting  to  read  it  a  second  time,  as  was  found  sometimes 
to  be  the  case  when  this  order  was  not  given.  He  is  also  told 
that  if  there  are  any  words  which  he  does  not  know,  if  he 
will  merely  point  to  them  they  will  be  pronounced  for  him. 
It  was  found  with  the  children  of  this  school  that  those  of  the 
second  grade  and  above  found  very  few  words  which  they 
could  not  pronounce. 


ii6  CLARA  SCHM1TT 

The  experimenter  uses  for  permanent  record  a  printed  sheet 
with  space  sufficiently  wide  so  that  changes  in  the  text  as 
rendered  by  the  child  may  be  written  in,  or  words  or  phrases 
omitted  in  his  rendering  may  be  crossed  out. 

Permanent  Record   Sheet  of  Test  XII 

If  a  man  finds  that  the  house  is  on  fire 

he  should  look  to  see  if  it  is  a  large  fire 

if  it  is  a  small  one 

he  should  pour  water  on  it 

or  smother  it 

but  if  it  is  large 

he  should  run  to  the  fire  alarm  box 

calling  out  fire 

to  the  other  people  in  the  house 

then  he  should  go  back 

and  help  old  or  sick  people 

and  little  children 

to  escape  from  the  burning  building 

when  all  the  people  are  out 

if  there  is  time 

he  may  save  valuable  things 

such  as  money  or  jewelry 

then  when  the  fire  engine  comes 

he  may  help  to  keep  the  crowds  of  curious  people  out  of  the  way 

so  that  the  firemen  may  work  more  easily. 

Table  XXVIII  shows  the  data,  arranged  according  to  grade, 
obtained  from  this  test.  The  data  are  the  number  of  items 
the  child  remembers.  What  is  considered  an  item  is  indicated 
by  the  length  of  line  in  the  record  sheet  above,  each  line  con- 
stituting one  item  of  the  passage.  A  judgment  is  made  in 
each  case  as  to  whether  the  child  was  verbally  accurate,  approxi- 
mately verbally  accurate,  or  made  no  attempt  to  be  verbally 
accurate.  The  correctness  of  the  sequence  of  items  is  also 
noted.  He  is  noted  in  the  column,  Sequence  correct  if  there 
is  not  more  than  one  detail  misplaced  in  the  selection.  He  is 
put  down  in  the  column,  Sequence  incorrect  if  more  than  one 
detail  is  misplaced  in  the  selection.  Following  is  an  example 
of  an  approximately  accurate  verbal  reproduction.  It  may  be 
remarked  here  that  there  were  none  absolutely  accurate. 

"If  a  man  finds  his  house  is  on  fire,  he  must  first  look  to 
see  if  it  is  a  large  fire.  If  it  is  a  small  fire,  he  should  pour 
water  on  it  to  smother  it;  but  if  it  is  large,  he  should  run  to 
the  fire  alarm  box  and  call  out  Tire!'  to  the  people.  Then 


STANDARDIZATION  OF  TESTS  117 

he  must  go  to  help  old  or  sick  people  out  from  the  fire.  Then, 
if  there  is  time,  he  may  bring  out  jewelry.  Then  when  the 
fire  engine  comes  he  may  help  to  keep  back  the  crowds  of  curi- 
ous people." 

Following  is  another  case,  which  was  put  in  the  column  No 
attempt  at  Verbal  Accuracy: 

"If  a  house  should  catch  on  fire,  and  the  man  should  see 
it,  he  should  look  to  see  if  it  is  a  large  fire.  If  it  is  a  small 
fire,  he  should  pour  water  on  it  or  smother  it;  but  if  he  finds 
it  is  large,  he  should  run  as  fast  as  he  could  to  the  fire  alarm 
box  and  ring  for  the  firemen.  Before  the  firemen  come,  if  the 
fire  isn't  very  bad  he  first  saves  the  sick  who  could  not  get  out. 
Next,  get  the  children  out,  and  when  the  firemen  come  keep 
the  crowds  back  so  that  the  firemen  can  work." 

Following  is  another  example  of  a  reproduction  placed  in 
the  column  No  Attempt  at  Verbal  Accuracy.  It  is  not  so  in- 
accurate as  the  preceding  one. 

"If  a  man  sees  a  fire,  he  must  first  look  to  see  if  it  was  a 
big  one  or  a  small  one.  If  it  is  a  small  one,  he  should  throw 
water  on  it;  but  if  it  is  a  big  one,  he  should  run  to  the  fire 
alarm  box  and  call  up  the  fire  engine.  Then  he  should  go 
back  and  see  if  he  can  do  anything  for  sick  or  helpless  people. 
After  all  the  people  are  taken  care  of  he  should  save  money 

TABLE  XXVIII 
Test   XII.     Memory    from  Visual   Verbal    Presentation 

Details   Remembered          Accuracy  Sequence 


19  14      Approx-  No  at-  Incor- 

to  15      or  less     imate      tempt  Correct      rect 


II  17  i  5  7  41  9  52  10  58  7  41  16  94  i  5 

III  21  i  4  12  57  8  38  19  90  2  9  19  90  2  9 

IV  24  o  o  15  62  9  37  23  95  i  4  23  95  I  4 
V  22  3  13  16  72  3  13  14  63  8  36  19  86  3  13 

VI  24  i  4  15  62  8  33  22  91  2  8  22  91  2  8 


ii8  CLARA  SCHMITT 

and  valuable  things.  Then  when  the  fire  engine  comes,  he 
may  help  to  keep  the  curious  people  away  from  the  fire,  so 
that  the  firemen  may  work  more  easily." 

The  data  of  Table  XXVII  show  that  beginning  with  the 
third  grade  more  than  61  per  cent  of  the  children  are  able 
to  recall  not  less  than  fifteen  items  of  the  twenty  they  have  read. 
These  data  are  in  striking  contrast  with  those  which  Binet 
obtained  with  his  visual  verbal  memory  test  of  the  1908  series. 
He  found  that  two  items  constitute  the  normal  for  eight-year- 
old  children.  The  data  which  Goddard  (15)  derived  from  the 
same  test  led  him  to  conclude  that  it  was  too  difficult  for  eight- 
year-old  children.  The  material  used  for  the  Binet  test  was 
as  follows: 

New  York,  September  5th.  A  fire  last  night'  burned  three 
houses  in  Water  Street.  It  took  some  time  to  put  it  out.  The 
loss  was  fifty  thousand  dollars,  and  seventeen  families  lost  their 
homes.  In  saving  a  girl  who  was  asleep  in  bed,  a  fireman 
was  burned  on  the  hand. 

The  difficulty  with  the  Binet  test  probably  lay  in  the  un- 
familiarity  of  much  of  its  material.  In  such  case  the  child's 
attention  is  often  so  much  engaged  with  the  matter  which  is 
unfamiliar  to  him  that  he  fails  to  organize  that  part  which  is 
familiar  to  him,  and  so  presents  the  appearance  of  failure  when 
such  is  not  really  the  case.  The  newspaper  type  of  beginning 
of  the  paragraph  could  only  confuse  a  non-newspaper  reading 
child;  the  unfamiliar  street  name,  and  the  unfamiliar  quantity, 
fifty  thousand,  followed  by  another  number  in  enumerating  the 
loss,  may  detract  attention  and  prevent  an  organization  of  the 
story  into  a  complete  whole. 

The  table  shows  that  all  the  children  were  approximately 
accurate  in  their  reproduction  of  the  selection  read,  and  that 
the  number  of  children  who  reproduced  the  selection  with  errors 
in  the  sequence  of  items  is  negligible  throughout,  with  the  excep- 
tion in  each  case  of  the  fifth  grade.  This  grade  presents  a 
rather  large  percentage  of  children  who  make  no  attempt  at 
verbal  accuracy  and  who  made  errors  in  the  sequence. 

Test  XIII.  Memory  from  Auditory  Verbal  Presentation.  The 
following  passage  is  read  to  the  child  four  times.  He  is  told 


STANDARDIZATION  OF  TESTS  119 

before  the  reading  that  he  must  listen  very  carefully  and  then  re- 
peat the  story  as  nearly  as  he  can  as  it  was  given  to  him,  but 
that  if  he  cannot  remember  it  precisely  he  should  give  it  as 
well  as  possible. 

If  a  sailor  on  the  ocean  is  shipwrecked  in  a  wild  country,  he  must  first 
look  for  water  to  drink;  then  he  must  find  a  place  to  sleep,  where  wild 
animals  can't  get  at  him ;  and  after  that  he  can  take  time  to  look  for  food, 
but  he  must  be  careful  not  to  eat  poisonous  berries  or  fruit.  Next,  he  had 
better  hunt  for  other  people  on  the  land,  and  put  up  a  flag  to  stop  ships  which 
may  be  going  by. 

Permanent  Record  Sheet  for  Test  XIII 
If  a  sailor 
on  the  ocean 
is  shipwrecked 
in  a  wild  country 

he  must  first  look  for  water  to  drink 
then  he  must  find  a  place  to  sleep 
where  wild  animals  won't  get  at  him 
and  after  that  he  can  take  time  to  look  for  food 
but  he  must  be  careful  not  to  eat  poisonous  berries  or  fruit 
next  he  had  better  hunt  for  other  people  on  the  land 
and  put  up  a  flag 
to  stop  ships  which  may  be  going  by. 

The  same  data  are  kept  for  this  passage  as  for  the  one  given 
above.  Following  is  an  example  of  a  passage  classed  as  No 
Attempt  at  Verbal  Accuracy: 

"If  a  sailor  is  shipwrecked,  he  has  to  be  careful  to  see  that 
he  has  water,  then  to  see  that  he  sleeps  where  wild  animals 
won't  get  at  him,  and  then  he  has  to  look  for  food,  and  be 
careful  not  to  eat  poisonous  berries  or  such  things,  and  then 
he  has  to  look  for  other  people,  and  put  up  a  flag  to  stop  ships 
going  by." 

The  following  is  an  example  of  a  reproduction  placed  in  the 
approximately  verbally  accurate  column : 

"If  a  sailor  on  the  ocean  is  shipwrecked  in  a  savage  land, 
he  must  first  look  for  water.  Next,  he  must  find  a  place  to 
sleep  where  wild  animals  won't  get  at  him.  Then  he  may 
look  for  food,  but  be  careful  that  he  does  not  eat  poisonous 
berries  or  fruit.  Next,  he  must  look  around  for  other  people 
on  the  land,  and  put  up  a  flag  to  stop  ships  going  by." 

Table  XXIX  shows  the  data  obtained  from  this  test : 


120  CLARA  SCHMITT 

TABLE  XXIX 
Test  XIII.     Memory  from  Auditory  Verbal  Presentation 

Details  iRemembered          Accuracy  Sequence 


ii  8       Approx-   No  at-  Incor- 

to  9       or  less    imate      tempt  Correct      rect 


1 

Grade 

Numiber 

Number 

^ 

Number 

* 

Number 

& 

Number 

* 

Number 

^ 

Number 

*R 

1 
£ 

^ 

I 

II 
III 
IV 
V 
VI 

13 

17 

21 

24 
22 

24 

i 

2 

4 
6 

9 
3 

7 

12 
19 
25 
31 
12 

10 
13 
16 
18 
13 
19 

77 
76 
76 
75 
59 
79 

2 
2 
I 
O 
O 
2 

15 

12 

5 

0 

o 
8 

5 
15 
20 

23 
16 

22 

38 

88 
95 
96 
73 
92 

8 

2 
I 
I 

6 

2 

61 

12 

5 
4 
3 
8 

3 
16 
20 
23 
19 
24 

23 
94 
95 
96 
86 

IOO 

10 

I 
I 
I 
3 

0 

77 
6 

5 
4 
13 
O 

It  may  be  observed  from  the  table  that  throughout  the  grades 
the  number  of  items  omitted  in  the  reproduction  are  negligible. 
The  children  in  general  are  able  to  remember  nine  or  more 
of  the  twelve  items  presented  them.  After  the  first  grade,  the 
percentages  of  children  who  do  not  attempt  to  be  verbally 
accurate  is  negligible,  and  after  the  first  grade  the  percentage  of 
those  who  do  not  get  the  sequence  correct  is  negligible. 

Test  VIII;  Learning  Test — Arbitrary  Associations. — The 
material  for  this  test  consists  of  nine  geometrical  figures  placed 
at  the  top  of  a  sheet  and  numbered.  Below  these  are  the 
figures  reproduced  each  one  three  times,  without  the  correspond- 
ing numbers;  below  these  is  another  row  of  the  geometrical 
figures  without  the  numbers.  Figure  9  is  a  reproduction  of  the 
sheet  for  this  test.  It  is  laid  before  the  child  and  he  is  told 
that  at  the  top  of  the  page  he  will  see  a  set  of  figures  or  little 
pictures  each  with  a  number,  and  that  below  are  the  same  kind 
of  figures  but  without  the  numbers,  in  which  he  is  to  place 
the  number  of  the  same  figure  in  the  top  row.  He  is  then 
asked,  "What  number  goes  in  the  first  figure?"  A  correct  re- 
sponse shows  his  understanding  of  his  task.  Many  children 
do  not  at  first  understand  the  instructions  and  begin  to  number 
the  figures  serially,  i,  2,  etc.  In  such  case  the  instructions  are 
repeated.  When  the  three  compact  lines  designed  for  that 
purpose  are  numbered  they  are  covered  and  the  child  is  told 


STANDARDIZATION  OF  TESTS 


121 


that  he  may  number  the  remainder  from  memory.  Healy  and 
Fernald  give  an  additional  instruction:  "When  he  has  done 
this  he  is  told  to  study  well  the  top  line  until  he  thinks  he  knows 
it."  This  procedure  is  not  followed  by  the  writer  because  it 
introduces  a  confusion  in  the  comparison  of  results.  Some 
children  take  much  time  and  some  take  only  a  glance  without 
attention  to  the  instructions.  This  case  is  a  clear  illustration 
cuf  the  fact  of  the  unreliability  of  time  measure  as  a  measure 
of  mental  process.  What  the  child  is  doing  mentally  in  what- 

FIG.    9 


A=*=l«_X©<? 


C 


X 

ALO  + 
O 


)AL 

xc  n 
c  x  = 


ever  time  he  does  take  can  only  be  guessed.  If  the  sheet  is 
covered  as  soon  as  the  last  figure  is  numbered  the  learning 
conditions  will  be  made  uniform.  This  test  is,  of  course,  only 
possible  after  the  child  has  gained  sufficient  practice  in  writing 
numerals  as  to  make  the  process  automatic  and  thus  free  the 
attention  from  this  part  of  the  task  to  that  of  fixing  the  de- 
sired associations.  The  test,  therefore  bears  some  relation  to 
the  curriculum.  It  can  not  be  given  until  after  such  time  as 
this  practice  has  been  gained  in  the  school.  Since  the  following 
table  shows  it  to  be  a  suitable  test  for  the  first  grade  child  it 


122  CLARA  SCHMITT 

would  probably  be  suitable  for  younger  children  if  it  were  de- 
vised in  a  form  which  would  do  away  with  the  necessity  for 
such  skill. 

The  scoring  takes  account  of  errors  of  perception — that  is 
the  errors  made  in  numbering  the  figures  from  the  model  line — 
and  the  errors  of  memory.  Table  XXX  shows  the  results  of 
this  test  for  the  first  four  grades. 

TABLE  XXX 

Test  VIII.    Learning  Test — Arbitrary  Associations-     (By  grade) 
Errors  of  Perception  Errors  of  Memory 

i  or  more  i  or  more 

No  errors  errors  No  errors  errors 


W 


'O 
rt 

s 

1 

1 

3 

1 

HH 

IH 

0 

£ 

* 

* 

* 

* 

^ 

i 

$ 

< 

I 

*20 

18 

90 

2 

IO 

II 

55 

9 

45 

2+ 

II 

17 

13 

76 

4 

23 

12 

70 

5 

29 

2+ 

III 

21 

21 

IOO 

o 

o 

17 

80 

4 

19 

IV 

22 

22 

IOO 

o 

o 

19 

86 

3 

13 

2+ 

*  Of  seventeen  first  grade  children  of  another  private  school,  none  made 
errors  of  perception,  two  made  two  errors  each  of  memory. 

The  table  shows  that  throughout,  the  errors  of  perception  are 
negligible  and  the  errors  of  memory  are  negligible  after  the 
second  grade.  The  average  error  of  those  who  made  errors 
are  not  more  than  two. 

It  may  be  noticed  that  in  the  line  for  memory  numbering  one 
of  the  figures  is  repeated.  This  forms  an  interesting  sort  of 
trap  for  the  unwary  type  of  mind  which  is  always  exhibited 
by  the  defective  individual.  This  type  of  person  will  give  it 
some  other  than  the  proper  number,  not  seeing  that  he  has  given 
different  numbers  to  the  same  figure;  or  when  he  comes  to  it, 
he  looks  over  his  past  work  and  finds  that  he  has  used  all  the 
numerals  up  to  ten  and  then  inserts  that.  The  normal  child 
often  shows  an  ability  to  help  his  memory  which  is  never  ex- 
hibited by  the  defective  individual.  When  the  former  has  for- 
gotten the  appropriate  number  for  one  figure  he  will  voluntarily 
or  upon  suggestion  go  on  and  number  the  others  which  he 
does  know;  then  he  will  look  over  his  work  again  and  find 


STANDARDIZATION  OF  TESTS  123 

which  numeral  he  has  not  yet  used  and  place  that  in  the  for- 
gotten figure.  Questioning  the  child  will  often  bring  out  the 
process  used  by  him  in  determining  the  forgotten  item.  An 
example  is  that  of  a  nine-year-old  boy.  He  was  asked,  "How 
did  you  know  that  was  the  right  number?"  "I  guessed." 
"How?"  "I  looked  and  saw  that  I  did  not  have  an  eight 
anywhere  and  I  knew  that  must  be  the  number."  The  defective 
individual  has  no  way  of  helping  himself  out  in  such  an 
emergency. 


VII 

CORRELATION  OF  THREE  TESTS  WITH 
SCHOOL  GRADE 

The  tests  which  have  been  discussed  above  were  the  ones 
chosen   for  standardization,   leaving  out   for  lack  of  time  the 
few  which  had  proved  of  least  value  in  the  work  of  the  clinic. 
In  order  to  show  what  correlation  exists  between  these  tests 
and  general  ability,   three  were  chosen  to  be  correlated  with 
school  standing  of  children  of  a  uniform  age.     School  standing 
may  be  taken  as  a  rough  measure  of  a  child's  general  ability 
to  learn,  because  of  the  varied  character  of  school  work  in  a 
city  school.     The  curriculum  contains  reading,  writing,  arith- 
metic, and  the  application  of  these  three  tools  in  the  acquisition 
of  organized  bodies  of  knowledge  such  as  history,  geography, 
etc. ;  and  it  contains  handwork  in  varying  degrees  of  complexity 
to  suit  the  various  grades.     In  the  Chicago  schools  a  child  is 
not  retained  in  a  grade  for  a  second  term  if  he  fails  in  no 
more  than  one  of  the  required  subjects  of  that  grade.     This 
rule  keeps  the  child  progressing  in  spite  of  the  lack  of  some 
one  specialized  ability  or  interest.     The  rule  can,  however,  be 
of  significance  only  for  the  grades  above  primary  work.     A 
child  who  can  not  learn  to  read,  for  instance,  could  make  little 
or  no  progress  from  grade  to  grade  since  all  the  work  of  the 
school  with  the  exception  of  handwork  rests  upon  reading  as  a 
foundation.     In  the  case  of  the  defective  child  school  grade 
may  not  indicate  a  true  measure  of  his  mental  ability, — at  least 
it  is  not  always  a  measure  of  his  accomplishments  in  the  work 
of  the  school.     Because  the  number  of  special  rooms  for  de- 
fective children  is  insufficient  for  the  supply  many  have  to  be 
kept  in  the  regular  grades.     When  a  defective  child  in  such 
circumstances  becomes  too  large  to  sit  in  the  seats  of  the  grade 
appropriate  to  his  school  accomplishments,  or  for  some  other 
reason,  he  is  sent  to  another  room  where  he  may  more  com- 


STANDARDIZATION  OF  TESTS  125 

fortably  be  taken  care  of.  It  is  seldom,  that  a  defective  child 
is  found  in  the  fifth  grade  or  above.  His  progress  through 
the  school  is  at  the  rate  of  two  years  or  more  to  a  grade  and 
at  the  age  of  fourteen  he  is  found  no  higher  than  the  fourth 
grade.  On  the  other  hand,  among  the  foreign  neighborhoods, 
many  children  are  in  low  grades  by  reason  of  late  entrance  into 
the  public  school.  A  foreign  born  child,  coming  to  America 
at  the  age  of  twelve,  must,  of  course,  be  placed  in  the  first 
or  second  grade  to  learn  to  read.  Other  children,  by  reason 
of  physical  handicaps,  may  be  in  grades  which  do  not  measure, 
perhaps,  innate  ability,  but  only  what  they  have  been  able  to 
accomplish  under  the  circumstances.  Poor  vision  or  hearing 
may  constitute  handicaps  to  learning  in  the  school,  when  the 
child's  innate  ability  to  learn  is  good.  The  school  grade  is 
not,  therefore,  an  exact  measure  of  mental  ability. 

With  the  above  qualifications  in  mind  the  following  tables 
may  be  taken  only  as  indications  of  an  existing  correlation  be- 
tween the  tests  chosen  for  the  purpose  and  general  ability  as 
indicated  by  the  school  grade.  The  tendency  to  positive  correla- 
tion is  clearly  shown. 

The  tests  which  have  for  prerequisites  the  smallest  amount 
of  formal  training  and  knowledge  were  selected  for  the  purpose 
of  correlation  with  school  grade  of  the  children  from  12-6  to 
13-6  seen  in  the  clinic.  All  of  these  children  came  from  public 
or  parochial  schools.  Children  of  this  age  who  have  begun 
school  at  the  legally  required  age  of  seven  and  have  progressed 
normally  through  the  school  a  grade  a  year  should  be  in  the 
seventh  grade.  Those  who  began  at  six,  the  legally  permissible 
age,  should  be  in  the  eighth  grade.  The  cases  were  scattered 
from  the  first  to  the  eighth. 

Table  XXXI  shows  the  data  obtained  from  Test  V,  the  puzzle 
box,  arranged  with  reference  to  the  qualitative  reaction  already 
discussed. 

The  table  shows  that  beginning  with  the  sixth  grade  prac- 
tically all  do  the  test  by  the  planned  method,  and  that  below 
the  fourth  grade  practically  all  fail.  Those  below  the  fifth 
grade  are  two  years  and  more  retarded  in  their  school  work. 


126  CLARA  SCHMITT 

TABLE  XXXI 
Test  V.     Puzzle  Box.     (Age  12-6  to  13-6.     Clinic  Cases) 


Method 


Failure  Trial  and  Error  Planned 

Grade          Number    Number          %          Number          %          Number          % 


I 

9 

7 

77 

2 

99 

o 

o 

II 

4 

3 

75 

I 

IOO 

o 

o 

III 

i 

i 

IOO 

O 

IOO 

o 

o 

IV 

8 

2 

25 

5 

87 

I 

12 

V 

5 

I 

20 

2 

60 

2 

40 

VI 

3 

O 

0 

I 

33 

2 

66 

VII 

i 

o 

O 

O 

o 

I 

IOO 

VIII 

2 

O 

0 

O 

0 

2 

IOO 

Total 

33 

14 

42 

II 

33 

8 

24 

A  similar  table  was  constructed  for  each  of  the  ages  from  n-6 
to  15-6,  the  years  for  which  the  grade  of  which  the  child  was 
a  member  in  the  school  or  at  which  he  quit  if  he  did  so  at  the 
legal  limit  of  fourteen  could  be  most  surely  ascertained.  These 
tables  showed  for  each  age  the  same  large  break  in  the  per- 
centages of  qualitative  reaction  to  the  test  at  the  two  year  re- 
tardation point  as  has  just  been  shown  in  the  table  for  the 
13-year-olds.  The  tables  were  then  combined  to  show  the  reac- 
tion for  two  year  or  more  retarded  cases  and  those  showing 
less  than  two  years  or  no  retardation.  Table  XXXII  shows  the 
data  so  arranged. 

TABLE  XXXII 

Test  V.    Puzzle  Box.     (n-6  to  15-6,  Retarded  and  Unretarded,  Clinic  Cases) 

Method 


Failure     Trial  &  Error     Planned 


i 


Retarded  2  yr.  or  more  115        46        40        57        89        12        10 

Unretarded  69         8        n        34        49        27        39 

The  table  shows  that  of  the  retarded  cases  10  per  cent  were 
able  to  plan  the  work  of  this  test,  and  of  the  unretarded  cases 
39  per  cent  were  able  to  plan  it;  that  40  per  cent  of  the  re- 
tarded failed  and  n  per  cent  of  the  unretarded  cases  failed. 


STANDARDIZATION  OF  TESTS  127 

Table  XXXIII  shows  the  data  obtained  for  the  12-6  to  13-6 
cases  for  Test  IX,  Cross  Line  A. 

TABLE  XXXIII 

Test  IX.     Cross  Line  A.     (Age  12-6  to  13-6.     Clinic  Cases) 

Succeeded 


Failure  First  Second  Third  Fourth 


1 

1 

V 

.Q 

1 

1 

J 

Grade 

1 

1 

* 

1 

* 

1 

* 

1 

* 

4 

* 

I 

9 

7 

77 

I 

II 

o 

o 

o 

o 

o 

o 

II 

4 

2 

50 

I 

25 

o 

o 

o 

o 

o 

o 

III 

3 

2 

66 

I 

33 

o 

o 

o 

o 

o 

o 

IV 

ii 

5 

45 

3 

27 

I 

9 

2 

18 

o 

o 

V 

6 

2 

33 

3 

50 

0 

o 

I 

16 

0 

0 

VI 

2 

O 

o 

2 

IOO 

o 

o 

o 

o 

o 

o 

VII 

I 

O 

o 

I 

IOO 

0 

o 

o 

0 

0 

o 

VIII 

2 

O 

o 

2 

IOO 

o 

o 

o 

o 

o 

o 

Total  38        18        47        14        36         3          7          3          7         o         o 

The  table  shows  that  above  the  fifth  grade  practically  all 
succeed  and  that  'below  the  fifth  grade  there  is  a  large  per- 
centage of  failure. 

The  data  of  this  test  were  arranged  with  reference  to  re- 
tardation as  was  the  test  last  discussed,  showing  success  and 
failure.  Table  XXXIV  shows  the  data  so  arranged. 

TABLE  XXXIV 

Test  IX.     Cross  Line  A.     (n-6  to   15-6,  Retarded  and  Unretarded,  Clinic 

Cases) 

Failure  Succeeded 


Number 

Number 

i 

r    " 

Number 

% 

Retarded  2  yr.  or  more 
Unretarded 

123 
79 

45 

7 

36 

8 

78 

72 

63 

Here  it  is  seen  that  of  the  retarded  cases  63  per  cent  succeeded 
and  of  the  unretarded  cases  91  per  cent  succeeded. 

The  data  for  test  X,  Cross  Line  B,  for  the  12-6  to  13-6  year 
cases  is  shown  in  Table  XXXV. 


128  CLARA  SCHMITT 


TABLE  XXXV 

Test  X.     Cross  Line  B.     (Age  12-6  to  13-6.    Clinic  Cases) 

Succeeded 


Failure 


First 


Second 


Third 


Fourth 


j? 

& 

J* 

.0 

JO 

•s 

Grade 

1 

| 

* 

* 

1 

£ 

z 

* 

1 

& 

I 

9 

7 

77 

i 

II 

0 

0 

I 

II 

o 

o 

II 

4 

3 

75 

o 

O 

o 

0 

o 

O 

i 

25 

III 

3 

2 

66 

o 

O 

I 

33 

o 

o 

0 

o 

IV 

10 

6 

60 

II 

IO 

3 

30 

o 

o 

o 

o 

V 

7 

i 

14 

3 

42 

2 

28 

I' 

14 

o 

o 

VI 

6 

o 

o 

5 

83 

0 

o 

I 

16 

o 

o 

VII 

i 

o 

o 

i 

IOO 

o 

o 

o 

o 

o 

o 

VIII 

2 

o 

0 

2 

IOO 

0 

o 

o 

0 

o 

o 

Total 

42 

19 

45 

12 

28 

6 

14 

2 

4 

I 

2 

•  This  table  also  shows  the  large  percentage  of  success  above 
the  fifth  grade  and  the  very  large  percentage  of  failure  below 
that  grade.  Table  XXXVI  shows  the  data  with  reference  to 
retarded  and  unretarded  cases. 

TABLE  XXXVI 
Test  X.    Cross  Line  B.    (11^6  to  15-6,  Retarded  and  Unretarded,  Clinic  Cases) 

Failure  Succeeded 

Number      Number          %          Number          % 


Retarded  2 
Unretarded 

yr. 

or 

more 

123 

84 

52 
4 

4 

80 

57 
95 

This  table  shows  that  57  per  cent  of  the  retarded  cases  and 
95  per  cent  of  the  unretarded  succeeded  with  this  test. 


VIII 

INDIVIDUAL  REACTIONS  TO  HEALY-FERNALD 

TESTS 

The  following  seven  tables  show  the  individual  data  of  the 
children  of  the  private  school  to  those  tests  which  involve  quali- 
tative types  of  reaction. 

The  first  column  to  the  left  shows  the  individual  number  of 
the  child  when  the  data  were  recorded  from  the  original  notes. 
The  next  column  records  age ;  the  third  grade,  and  the  following 
columns  the  type  of  reaction  to  each  test. 


TABiLE  XXXVII 
Reaction   of  Kindergarten   Children  to   Healy-Fernald   Tests 


No. 
157 
158 
140 
i'5S 
145 
154 
138 
160 
141 
159 
149 
132 
139 
137 
144 

143 
153 
135 
142 
140 
152 
134 
151 
136 
147 
148 


Age    Grade 

5+     Kdg. 

5  + 

5-  3 

5-8 

5-  9 

5-  9 

5-10 

5-n 

6 

6-  i 

6-  i 

6-  2 

6-  2 

6-  3 

6-  4 

6-  5 

6-  6 

6-6 

6-  6 

6-10 

7 

7-  4 

7-10 

8-  i 

Test  III 

Chance,  Learning  -j- 
Tr.  and  Er.,  Learning 
Tr.  and  Er.,  Learning 
Chance,  Learning  -|- 
Planned 

Chance,  Learning  -f- 
Chance,  Learning  -\- 
Tr.  and  Er.,  Learning 
Chance,  Learning  + 
Chance,  Learning  -j- 
Chance,  Learning  -j- 
Chance,  Learning  -j- 
Planned 
Planned 

Tr.  and  Er.,  Learning 
Tr.  and  Er.,  Learning 
Chance,  Learning  -f- 
Tr.  and  Er.,  Learning 
Chance,  Learning  + 
Chance,  Learning  -j- 
Chance,  Learning  -j- 
Planned 

Chance,  Learning  -f- 
Planned 

Chance,  Learning  -f- 
Chance,  Learning  -j- 


-f-  Method  for  second  attempt,  Trial  and 
*  Made  errors,  but  method  planned. 


Test  IV 

Chance,  Readjustment  — •* 
-f-      Chance,  Readjustment  -|- 
-j-      Planned,  Readjustment  -f- 
Chance,  Readjustment  — * 
Planned,  Readjustment  -{- 
Planned,  Readjustment  -j- 
Chance,  Readjustment  -(- 
-f-      Planned,  Readjustment  -f- 
Tr.  and  Er.,  Readjustment  -f- 
Chance,  Readjustment  — * 
Tr.  and  Er.,  Readjustment  -f- 
Chance,  Readjustment  -(- 
Planned,  Readjustment  + 
Tr.  and  Er.,  Readjustment  -(- 
+      Chance,  Readjustment  + 
-f-      Planned,  Readjustment  -(- 
Planned,  Readjustment  + 
+     'Chance,  Readjustment  — * 
Planned,  Readjustment  + 
Chance,  Readjustment  -{- 
Tr.  and  Er.,  Readjustment  + 
Planned,  Readjustment  + 
Chance,  Readjustment  + 
Planned,  Readjustment  + 
Planned,  Readjustment  + 
Tr.  and  Er.,  Readjustment  -f- 
Error. 


130 


CLARA  SCHMITT 


TABLE  XXXVIII 
Reaction  of  First  Grade  Children  to  Healy-Fernald  Tests 


Test 

No. 

Age  Grade     Test  III    Test  IV 

Test  V 

Test  IX    X 

93 

6-6      I      Planned       Planned 

Tr.  and  Er. 

ISt 

ISt 

39 

6-6 

Chance         Planned 

F* 

F 

F 

92 

6-6 

Planned       Chance 

Tr.  and  Er. 

ust 

3rd 

96 

6-7 

Tr.  and  Er.Tr.  and  Er. 

F 

ISt 

ISt 

91 

7 

Chance         Chance 

F 

F 

F 

94 

7-i 

Planned       Planned 

F 

ISt 

F 

127 

7-1 

Chance         Planned 

F 

3rd 

3rd 

86 

7-2 

Chance         Chance 

Tr.  and  Er. 

ISt 

ISt 

129 

7-4 

Chance        Tr.  and  Er. 

F 

I9t 

ISt 

98 

7-5 

Chance         Chance 

Tr.  and  Er. 

ISt 

F 

88 

7-5 

Planned       Chance 

Tr.  and  Er. 

ISt 

4th 

128 

7-5 

Chance        Tr.  and  Er. 

F 

3rd 

ISt 

90 

7-7 

Tr.  and  Er.Chance 

F 

ISt 

F 

87 

7-10 

Chance        Chance 

F 

ISt 

ISt 

97 

8 

Chance         Chance 

Tr.  and  Er. 

ISt 

2nd 

95 

8 

Tr.  and  Er.Chance 

Tr.  and  Er. 

ISt 

ISt 

131 

8 

Tr.  and  Er.Planned 

Tr.  and  Er. 

4th 

F 

125 

8 

Chance         Tr.  and  Er. 

F 

4th 

2nd 

126 

8-2 

Chance         Planned 

F 

ISt 

ISt 

89 

8-6 

Planned        Chance 

Tr.  and  Er. 

ISt 

ISt 

*F  =  Failure 


Reaction  of  Second  Grade  Children  to  Healy-Fernald  Tests 

No.  Age  Grade  Test  III            Test  IV  Test  V    Test  IX  Test  X  Test  XI 

32  7-6  II  Tr.  andEr.  Chance  F  ist  ist  Idea  — 

36  7-7  "  Chance  Chance  Tr.  and  Er.  ist  ist  Idea  — 

29  7-8  "  Tr.  andEr.  Planned  Tr.  and  Er.  ist  1st  Idea  — 

37  7-8  "  Planned  Planned  Tr.  and  Er.  ist  ist  Idea  — 

38  7-8  Tr.  and  Er.  Planned  Tr.  and  Er.  F  F 

27  7-9  "  Tr.  andEr.  Planned  Tr.  andEr.  ist  1st  Idea  — 

33  7-9  "  Tr.  and  Er.  Planned  Tr.  and  Er.  ist  F 

30  7-10  "  Planned  Chance  F  ist  ist  Idea  + 

34  7-10  "  Planned  Planned  Tr.  and  Er.  ist  2nd  Idea  -j- 

31  7-n  "  Chance  Planned  Tr.  and  Er.  ist  ist  Idea  — 

35  8  Planned  Planned  Tr.  and  Er.  ist  2nd  Idea  — 

28  8-2  "  'Chance  Planned  Tr.  and  Er.  2nd  ist  Idea  — 

23  8-7  "  Planned  Planned  Tr.  and  Er.  ist  ist  Idea  — 

25  8-8  Planned  Planned  Tr.  and  Er.  F  4th  Idea  + 

24  8-9  Planned  Planned  Tr.  and  Er.  ist  ist  Idea  — 

26  8-10  '  Planned  Chance  Planned  ist  4th  Idea  — 
22  9-3  "  Planned  Tr.  and  Er.  Tr.  and  Er.  ist  4th  Idea  — 


STANDARDIZATION  OF  TESTS 


TABLE  X'L 
Reaction   of   Third   Grade    Children   to   Healy-Fernald    Tests 


131 


No. 

Age  Grade  Test  III 

Test  IV 

Test  V    Test  IX 

Test  X 

Test 

XI 

20 

8       III     Chance 

Planned 

Tr.  and  Er. 

ISt 

ret 

Idea 

— 

21 

8-2 

Planned 

•Chance 

Tr.  and  Er. 

.ISt 

2nd 

Idea 

_|_ 

6 

8-8 

Planned 

Planned 

Tr.  and  Er. 

4th 

ISt 

Idea 

— 

12 

8-9 

Tr.  and  Er. 

Chance 

Tr.  and  Er. 

ISt 

1st 

Idea 

-|- 

IO 

8-10 

Tr.  and  Er. 

Planned 

Tr.  and  Er. 

ISt 

ISt 

Idea 

— 

2 

9 

Planned 

Chance 

F 

ISt 

ISt 

Idea 

— 

3 

9 

Planned 

Planned 

F 

ISt 

ISt 

p 

13 

9-2 

Chance 

Chance 

Tr.  and  Er. 

ISt 

ISt 

Idea 

-j- 

5 

9-2 

Planned 

Planned 

F 

ISt 

ISt 

Idea 

— 

7 

9-2 

Chance 

Chance 

Tr.  and  Er. 

ISt 

ISt 

? 

8 

9-2 

Planned 

Planned 

Planned 

ISt 

ISt 

Idea 

— 

I 

9-3 

Planned 

Chance 

Tr.  and  Er. 

ISt 

ISt 

Idea 

-f- 

ii 

9-4 

Tr.  and  Er. 

Planned 

Tr.  and  Er. 

ISt 

ISt 

Idea 

-j- 

4 

9-4 

Planned 

Planned 

Planned 

ISt 

ISt 

Idea 

-j- 

9 

9-4 

Tr.  and  Er. 

Planned 

Tr.  and  Er. 

ISt 

ISt 

Idea 

4- 

17 

9-9 

Planned 

Planned 

Tr.  and  Er. 

ISt 

I'St 

Idea 

-j- 

16 

9-9 

Planned 

Chance 

Tr.  and  Er. 

ISt 

ISt 

Idea 

4- 

18 

9-II 

Chance 

Tr.  and  Er. 

Tr.  and  Er. 

ISt 

3rd 

Idea 

— 

14 

IO-2 

Planned 

Planned 

Tr.  and  Er. 

ISt 

ISt 

Idea 

-|- 

15 

10-4 

Tr.  and  Er. 

Planned 

Tr.  and  Er. 

ISt 

3rd 

Idea 

— 

19 

10-4            Tr.  and  Er. 

Planned 

Tr.  and  Er. 

ISt 

ISt 

Idea 

4- 

TABLE  XLI 

Reaction   of   Fourth 

Grade    Children   to    Healy-Fernald 

Tests 

No. 

Age  Grade  Test  III 

Test  IV 

Test  V    Test  IX 

Test  X 

Test 

XI 

41 

9        IV     Planned 

Planned 

Tr.  and  Er. 

ISt 

ISt 

Idea 

4- 

42 

9-4              Tr.  and  Er. 

Planned 

Tr.  and  Er. 

ISt 

ISt 

Idea 

4- 

43 

9-4              Tr.  and  Er. 

Planned 

Tr.  and  Er. 

4th 

ISt 

Idea 

-j- 

40 

9-5       "       Planned 

Planned 

Tr.  and  Er. 

ISt 

3rd 

Idea 

— 

56 

9-9      "       Planned 

Planned 

F 

I'St 

ISt 

Idea 

4- 

53 

9-9              Planned 

Planned 

Planned 

ISt 

ISt 

Idea 

-j- 

47 

9-9              Tr.  and  Er. 

Planned 

Planned 

ISt 

ISt 

Idea 

— 

51 

9-10 

Planned 

Planned 

Tr.  and  Er. 

ISt 

ISt 

Idea 

4- 

48 

9-10 

'      Planned 

Tr.  and  Er. 

Planned 

ISt 

ISt 

Idea 

— 

45 

10 

Tr.  and  Er. 

Planned 

F 

ISt 

4th 

Idea 

— 

50 

IO-I 

Planned 

Planned 

Planned 

ISt 

ISt 

Idea 

4- 

55 

IO-2 

Tr.  and  Er. 

Planned 

Tr.  and  Er. 

4th 

3rd 

Idea 

4- 

52 

IO-2 

Tr.  and  Er. 

Chance 

Tr.  and  Er. 

ISt 

I'St 

Idea 

-j- 

46 

1  0-4 

'       Planned 

Chance 

Tr.  and  Er. 

ISt 

4th 

Idea 

— 

49 

10-4 

Planned 

Planned 

Tr.  and  Er. 

ISt 

ISt 

Idea 

— 

54 

10-5 

Planned 

Planned 

Tr.  andEr. 

ISt 

I'St 

Idea 

4. 

57 

10-5 

'       Planned 

Planned 

Tr.  and  Er. 

ISt 

ISt 

? 

44 

10-5 

'      Planned 

Planned 

Tr.  and  Er. 

ISt 

ISt 

Idea 

— 

61 

10-8 

'       Planned 

Planned 

Tr.  and  Er. 

2nd 

4th 

Idea 

— 

59 

10-9 

'       Planned 

Planned 

Tr.  and  Er. 

4th 

3rd 

Idea 

4. 

60 

IO-9 

'      Planned 

Planned 

Planned 

ISt 

ISt 

Idea 

-j- 

58 

II-2 

'       Planned 

Planned 

Planned 

ISt 

ISt 

Idea 

— 

63 

11-6 

'       Planned 

Planned 

Planned 

ISt 

ISt 

Idea 

4, 

62 

12-2     '      Planned 

Chance 

Tr.  and  Er. 

ISt 

ISt 

Idea 

4- 

132  CLARA  SCHM1TT 

TAB'LE  XLII 
Reaction   of   Fifth   Grade   Children   to    Healy-Fernald    Tests 


No. 

Age  Grade  Test  III 

Test  IV 

Test  V    Test  IX 

Test  X 

Test  XI 

66 

10-3     V 

Planned 

Chance 

Planned 

ISt 

rst 

Idea  + 

65 

10-3    ' 

Planned 

Tr.  and  Er. 

Tr.  and  Er. 

ISt 

ISt 

Idea   + 

64 

10-5     ' 

Planned 

Planned 

Planned 

ISt 

ISt 

Idea   — 

67 

10-6 

Planned 

Planned 

Planned 

ISt 

ISt 

Idea   — 

69 

10-6     ' 

Planned 

Planned 

Planned 

ISt 

ISt 

Idea   + 

72 

ii 

Planned 

Planned 

Planned 

ISt 

ISt 

Idea   + 

7i 

1  1-2 

Planned 

Planned 

Planned 

ISt 

rst 

Idea   -\- 

73 

1  1-2       ' 

Chance 

Planned 

Tr.  and  Er. 

rst 

2nd 

Idea   — 

68 

1  1-5    " 

Planned 

Planned 

Planned 

ISt 

rst 

Idea   + 

70 

n-5    ' 

Chance 

Planned 

Planned 

rst 

rst 

Idea   + 

74 

11-6    " 

Chance 

Planned 

Tr.  and  Er. 

ISt 

2nd 

Idea   + 

78 

1  1-6    " 

Planned 

Chance 

Tr.  and  Er. 

ISt 

I'St 

Idea   + 

79 

1  1-8    " 

Planned 

Planned 

Planned 

ISt 

ISt 

Idea   + 

77 

12 

Planned 

Tr.  and  Er. 

Planned 

ISt 

ISt 

Idea  + 

76 

I2-I      " 

Chance 

Planned 

Tr.  and  Er. 

ISt 

2nd 

Idea   — 

75 

12-2      " 

Planned 

Planned 

Planned 

ISt 

ISt 

? 

83 

12-6      " 

Planned 

Chance 

Tr.  and  Er. 

ISt 

rst 

Idea   + 

80 

12-7    " 

Tr.  and  Er. 

Planned 

Tr.  and  Er. 

ISt 

rst 

Idea   — 

82 

l!2-7      " 

Planned 

Tr.  and  Er. 

Tr.  and  Er. 

ISt 

3rd 

Idea    + 

84 

12-11    " 

Chance 

Chance 

Planned 

ISt 

ISt 

Idea   — 

81 

12-  I  I    " 

Planned 

Planned 

Tr.  and  Er. 

ISt 

ISt 

Idea   + 

85 

13-8     " 

Planned 

Planned 

Planned 

ISt 

ISt 

Idea   + 

TABLE  XLIII 
Reaction  of  Sixth  Grade  Children  to  Healy4Fernald  Tests 


No. 

Age  Grade  Test  III 

Test  IV 

Test  V    Test  IX 

Test  X 

Test  XI 

US 

12      VI     Planned 

Planned 

Planned 

ISt 

ISt 

Idea   — 

116 

12               Planned 

Planned 

Planned 

rst 

ISt 

Idea  4- 

124 

12 

Planned 

Planned 

Planned 

ISt 

ISt 

Idea   4- 

105 

I2-I 

Chance 

Tr.  and  Er. 

Planned 

ISt 

ISt 

Idea  — 

109 

I2-I 

Planned 

Planned 

Planned 

rst 

rst 

Idea  4- 

104 

12-1 

Planned 

Planned 

Planned 

ISt 

ISt 

Idea   — 

123 

12-1 

Planned 

Planned 

Planned 

rst 

rst 

Idea   4- 

rn 

12-2 

Tr.  and  Er. 

Planned 

Tr.  and  Er. 

ISt 

ISt 

Idea   -j- 

IOO 

12-2 

Planned 

Tr.  and  Er. 

Planned 

ISt 

ISt 

Idea   — 

114 

12-3 

Planned 

Planned 

Tr.  and  Er. 

ISt 

I'St 

Idea   4" 

1  02 

12-6 

Tr.  and  Er. 

Planned 

Planned 

ISt 

ISt 

Idea   — 

106 

12-6 

Planned 

Planned 

Planned 

ISt 

ISt 

Idea   4- 

119 

12-8 

Planned 

Tr.  and  Er. 

Planned 

ust 

ISt 

Idea   — 

118 

12-8 

Tr.  and  Er. 

Chance 

Tr.  and  Er. 

ISt 

ISt 

Idea   4- 

113 

12-8 

Chance 

Planned 

Planned 

ISt 

rst 

p 

108 

12-9 

Planned 

Planned 

Planned 

ust 

ISt 

Idea   — 

103 

12-9 

Planned 

Planned 

Planned 

ISt 

ISt 

Idea   — 

1  20 

I2-III 

Tr.  and  Er. 

Planned 

Tr.  and  Er. 

ISt 

ISt 

Idea   — 

99 

13 

Planned 

Planned 

ISt 

ISt 

Trlpa 

122 

13 

Planned 

Planned 

Planned 

ISt 

ISt 

Idea   4- 

121 

13 

Tr.  and  Er. 

Planned 

Tr.  and  Er. 

ISt 

ISt 

Idea   -j- 

117 

13-4 

Tr.  and  Er. 

Chance 

Tr.  and  Er. 

rst 

rst 

Idea   — 

no 

13-5 

Planned 

Planned 

Planned 

ISt 

rst 

Idea   -f- 

107 

14-2            Planned 

Tr.  and  Er. 

Tr.  and  Er. 

ISt 

ISt 

Idea   + 

IX 

SUMMARY  OF  STANDARDIZATION  OF  HEALY- 
FERNALD  TESTS 

From  the  tables  and  discussions  above  may  be  summarized 
the  reaction  to  be  expected  of  children  of  different  ages  to  each 
test. 

Test  I.  Introductory  Puzzle. — Accomplished  by  children  of 
kindergarten  age  or  experience.  Eighty-nine  per  cent  make  less 
than  five  errors  in  performing  the  test  exclusive  of  the  triangle. 
Triangle  constitutes  a  learning  test. 

Test  II.  Special  Picture  Puzzle. — At  the  age  of  9-6  to  10-6 
accomplished  by  75  per  cent  with  not  more  than  two  errors. 

Test  III.  Construction  Puzzle  A. — At  the  age  of  10-6,  90 
per  cent  do  the  test  by  the  planned  method.  Under  the  age  of 
8-6  is  done  by  a  large  percentage  by  chance.  Constitutes  a  learn- 
ing test  for  children  of  kindergarten  age. 

Test  IF.  Construction  Puzzle  B. — At  the  age  of  8-6  to  9-6, 
65  per  cent  of  the  children  do  the  test  by  the  planned  method. 
Constitutes  a  learning  test  and  a  test  of  readjustment  of  an 
already  learned  content  to  fit  an  altered  situation  for  children 
of  kindergarten  age. 

Test  V.  Puzzle  Box.. — At  the  age  of  10-6  to  n-6  the  test 
is  performed  by  the  planned  method  by  72  per  cent  of  the 
children  tested. 

Test  IX.  Cross  Line  Test  A. — Performed  by  75  per  cent 
of  children  of  first  grade  experience  (so  graded  because  of 
necessity  of  accomplishment  of  writing1  numbers) — with  not 
more  than  two  trials. 

Test  X.     Cross  Line  Test  B. — Performed  by  69  per  cent  of 
children  of  second  grade  with  not  more  than  two  trials.     Per- 
formed by  65  per  cent  of  children  of  first  grade  with  not  more 
than  three  trials. 
•  Test  XI.     Code  Test. — The  reasoning  step  is  performed  by 


134  CLARA  SCHMITT 

60  per  cent  of  children  of  fourth  grade.  At  the  fourth  grade 
and  above  the  average  error  is  not  more  than  four  out  of  the 
possible  eleven. 

Test  XV.  Opposite  Test. — Performed  by  62  per  cent  of 
children  between  6-6  and  7-6  with  not  more  than  two  errors 
out  of  a  possible  twenty. 

Test  XII.  Memory  for  Visual  Verbal  Presentation. — Sixty-one 
per  cent  of  children  of  third  grade  recall  not  less  than  fifteen  items 
of  the  twenty.  Is  not  suitable  for  children  under  second  grade 
because  of  necessity  of  reading  accomplishment. 

Test  XIII.  Memory  from  Auditory  Verbal  Presentation. — 
Eighty-four  per  cent  of  first  grade  children  recall  not  less  than 
nine  items  of  the  twelve. 

Test  Fi/77.  Learning  Test,  Arbitrary  Associations. — Per-i 
formed  by  55  per  cent  of  children  of  first  grade  with  no  error, 
and  by  45  per  cent  with  average  of  two  errors;  by  70  per  cent 
of  second  grade  children  with  no  errors. 

The  above  summary  may  be  arranged  with  reference  to  grade. 
After  each  test  in  the  summary  below  is  indicated  the  reaction 
to  be  expected  of  the  children  of  the  grade  under  which  the 
test  is  listed.  The  type  of  reaction  shown  by  approximately 
75  per  cent  of  the  children  of  the  grade  is  indicated  as  the  one 
to  be  expected  of  the  grade.  In  case  this  percentage  is  dis- 
tributed over  two  or  more  types  of  reaction  these  types  are 
indicated  with  the  one  which  is  preferable  mentioned  first. 
Where  a  test  has  reached  a  maximum  in  one  grade  or  which 
shows  little  variation  for  several  grades  it  is  not  repeated  in 
the  summary  for  the  higher  grades. 

Kindergarten 

Test  I.  Errors  with  paired  pieces.  Triangle  by  trial  and 
error,  learning  test. 

Test  II.  Accomplish  (in  contrast  with  defectives  of  same 
age)  with  much  trial  and  error. 

Test  III.    Trial  and  error  or  chance,  learning  test. 

Test  IV.     Trial  and  error  or  chance,   learning  test. 


STANDARDIZATION  OF  TESTS  135 

Grade  I 

Test  I.       Less  than  three  errors.    Triangle  by  trial  and  error. 
Test  II.    Errors  of  paired  pieces. 
Test  III.  Trial  and  error  or  chance. 
Test-  IV.  Trial  and  error  or  chance. 
Test  V.     Failure. 

Test  IX.  Succeed  with  second  trial. 
Test  X.     Succeed  first  to  fourth  trial. 

Test  XV.  Succeed  (in  contrast  with  defectives)  with  less  than 
four  errors. 

Test  XIII.  Recall  nine  or  more  items. 
Test  Fill.  Two  errors  of  memory. 

Grade  II 

Test  I.       Triangle,  trial  and  error  or  planned. 

Test  III.  Trial  and  error  or  planned. 

Test  IV.  Planned  or  trial  and  error. 

Test  V.     Trial  and  error. 

Test  IX.  Succeed  first  trial. 

Test  X.     Succeed  first  to  third  trial. 

Test  XI.    Idea — ,  eight  errors. 

Test  XV.  Less  than  three  errors. 

Test  XII.  Twelve  or  more  items.     (In  the  column,  14  or  less.} 

Table  XXVIII,  p.  117,  are  included  three  children  who  remem- 
bered less  than  twelve  items.) 
Test  XIII.  Nine  or  more  items. 

Grade  III 

Test  III.  Planned  or  trial  and  error. 

Test  IV.  Planned  or  trial  and  error. 

Test  V.     Trial  and  error. 

Test  IX.  Succeed  first  trial. 

Test  X.     Succeed  first  trial. 

Test  XI.  Idea  +  or  — ,  seven  errors. 

Test  XV.  Three  or  less  errors. 


136  CLARA  SCHMITT 

Grade  IV 
Test  III.  Planned. 
Test  IF.  Planned. 
Test  V.     Trial  and  error. 
Test  XL  Idea  -f- ,  four  errors. 

Grade   V 

Test  V .     Planned  or  trial  and  error. 
Test  XL  Idea  +  ,  two  errors. 


X 

SCHOOL  SUBJECTS  AS  MATERIAL  FOR  TESTS  OF 
MENTAL  ABILITY 

In  several  large  cities  the  school  child,  because  of  his  un- 
favorable reactions  to  the  school  situation,  comes  in  for  clinical 
diagnosis  of  mental  and  physical  condition.  Since  it  is  the 
child's  reaction  to  the  school  situation  which  is  at  fault,  it  is 
well  to  test  him  along  the  line  of  the  special  abilities  which  he 
is  expected  to  develop  under  the  conditions  of  the  school  situa- 
tion. The  school  subjects  may  be  made  to  form  a  series  of 
tests  which  can  be  used  from  year  to  year  to  measure  or  check 
up  the  development  of  special  abilities.  The  curriculum  of  the 
school  forms  a  serial  arrangement  of  accomplishments  proceed- 
ing from  the  simplest  subject-matter  of  the  first  grade  to  the 
complexities  of  the  eighth  grade.  Such  an  arrangement  of  tests 
derived  from  the  school  subjects,  as  forms  a  psychological  serial 
arrangement  from  that  which  is  simplest  to  that  which  is  com- 
plex may  be  derived  from  the  curriculum  as  it  exists.  The 
following  series  of  tests  and  suggestions  for  the  evaluation 
of  the  child's  development  with  reference  to  the  school  curric- 
ulum has  resulted  from  an  examination  of  several  hundred 
children  considered  by  the  school  to  be  unfavorable  in  their 
reaction  to  the  school  situation,  and  a  comparison  of  them  with 
children  considered  normal  with  regard  to  their  reaction  to 
the  school  situation.  The  subjects  chosen  for  this  series  of 
tests  are  those  of  reading,  writing  and  arithmetic. 

Reading. — The  most  important  accomplishment  in  the  school 
life  is  that  of  reading.  The  child's  progress  throughout  the 
school  is  dependent  entirely  upon  his  attaining  it.  Upon  it 
depends  his  progress  to  a  large  extent  in  arithmetic  and  almost 
entirely  in  history  and  geography  and  other  such  subjects  which 
consist  of  classified  or  organized  groups  of  facts.  The  ac- 
complishment of  the  child  in  this  subject  may  be  arranged  with 


138  CLARA  SCHMITT 

reference  to  quantity  and  quality.  A  defective  child  may  be 
deficient  in  one  or  both  of  these  two  characteristics  of  the 
reading  accomplishment.  He  may  be  ^incapable  of  learning  to 
recognize  the  words  of  the  printed  page;  he  may  show  himself 
capable  of  learning  words  only  very  slowly  or  of  forgetting 
them  quickly  and  easily;  he  may  show  himself  capable  of  learn- 
ing words  with  some  facility  in  memorizing  them,  and  so  of 
becoming  a  good  reader,  but  incapable  of  gaining  ideas  from 
the  words  which  he  reads.  It  is  this  latter  characteristic  which 
one  is  to  understand  as  included  in  its  various  aspects  under  the 
term  "quality." 

The  child  may  show  an  ability  to  recognize  words  from 
the  printed  page  to  a  greater  or  less  extent,  but  this  recognition 
with  the  defective  child  consists  largely,  merely  of  a  mechanical 
type  of  visual  memory  which  serves  as  stimulus  for  its  associated 
vocal  prototype.  The  child  who  learns  words  in  this  way  only 
is  always  dependent  upon  his  teacher,  since  he  can  acquire  for 
himself  no  new  or  unfamiliar  word  from  the  printed  page. 
He  can  become  somewhat  independent  of  his  teacher  only  if 
he  learns  phonetic  values.  Defective  children  are  sometimes 
capable  of  acquiring  very  large  visual  vocabularies  but  show 
themselves  quite  deficient  in  perceiving  phonetic  relationships. 
Children  of  the  first  grade  may  be  expected  to  acquire  the 
simplest  phonetic  elements  of  the  English  language.*  The  child 
who  can  obtain  a  visual  vocabulary  with  facility,  who  gains  a 
perception  of  the  simple  phoentic  values,  and  who  learns  to 
combine  them  correctly  for  the  independent  learning  of  new 
words  is  considered  a  favorable  reactor  so  far  as  the  subject 
of  reading  of  the  first  grade  is  concerned  in  the  public  schools. 
The  various  steps  from  the  early  period  of  the  reading  ac- 
complishment to  its  complex  fulfillment  are  indicated  as  follows : 

I.    QUANTITY 

i.  Knows  no  words. — This  is  the  condition  of  the  average 
child  when  he  enters  school  at  six  or  seven  years  of 
age,  and  is  one  persisted  in  by  the  low  type  of  defective 

*  Cf.  Chicago  Public  Schools,  Course  of  Study  for  the  Elementary  Schools, 
1912. 


STANDARDIZATION  OF  TESTS  139 

child  for  several  years  or  longer.  This  low  type  of  defective 
child  shows  himself  incapable  of  perceiving  the  fine  differences 
which  serve  to  distinguish  one  word  from  another  on  the  printed 
page,  though  he  is  able  to  use  spoken  language.  Some  knowledge 
of  the  degree  of  his  defectiveness  may  be  gained  when  one 
knows  the  length  of  time  in  which  he  has  persisted  in  this 
disability. 

2.  Can  recognize  a  few  unrelated  words. — This   is   the   ac- 
complishment of  the  average  normal  child  after  a   few   days 
spent  in  the  school.     It  is  a  condition  persisted   in  by  many 
defective  children  sometimes  for  years.     In  such  case,  the  de- 
fective child  has  learned  a  word  here  and  a  word  there  which 
have  stuck  in  his  memory,   and  he  recognizes  them  wherever 
he  sees  them.     He  shows  himself,  however,  incapable  of  gain- 
ing sufficient  words  to  make  his  reading  a  consecutive  process 
with  regard  to  meaning.     The  words  which  he  does  learn  bear, 
perhaps,  no  relation  to  the  amount  or  type  of   teaching  that 
has  been  given.     The  learning  of  them  is  largely  a  matter  of 
chance,   and   just  why   certain  words   have   been   learned   and 
many  others  imparted  at  the  same  time  in  his  instruction  have 
been  forgotten  cannot  be  determined. 

3.  Can  read  entire  sentence  in  the  first  or  some  other  reader. — 
This  step  in  its  simplest   form  is   attained  by  the  child   after 
a  few  weeks  in  school.     The  reader  which  he  has  in  school, — 
if  because  of  being  a  defective  he   is  placed  in  an  ungraded 
room,' — compared  with  the  number  of  years  that  the  child  has 
been  in  school  is  some  measure  of  his  defectiveness  in  learning 
to  read. 

4.  Can  read  at  sight  any  material  such  as  newspapers,  etc. — 
This  is  the  highest  grade  which  may  be  attained  in  the  ability 
to  read,  with  reference  to  quantity.     It  is  attained  by  the  normal 
child  with  the  fifth  grade. 

The  phonetics  which  underlie  the  reading  process  is  the  greai 
stumbling  block  of  the  defective  child.  Seldom  is  one  found  who 
has  this  accomplishment.  He  may  be  able  to  learn  a  very  few 
of  the  simplest  combinations,  such  as  consist  of  one  or  two 
consonants  and  a  vowel.  The  normal  child  progresses  in  his 


140  CLARA  SCHMITT 

knowledge  of  phonetic  values,  to  such  an  extent  that  he  becomes 
independent  of  the  teacher  in  so  far  as  the  illogical  complexities 
of  our  English  spelling  permit.  At  the  fourth  grade  the  normal 
child  is  able  to  work  out  new  and  unfamiliar  words  with  approxi- 
mate phonetic  correctness. 

II.    QUALITY 

i.  Mechanical. — The  defective  child  may  be  able  to  ac- 
complish with  reference  to  quantity  in  reading  anything 
between  the  limits  set  above  from  the  lowest  to  the  highest 
stage  of  accomplishment.  However  great  his  accomplishment 
in  the  quantity  of  his  reading  he  is  unable  to  read  a  new  passage 
other  than  mechanically,  that  is  all  he  can  do  is,  to  use  a 
familiar  popular  phrase,  parrot-like.  This  type  of  reading  may 
be  described  as  a  straight  line  association  between  the  visual 
and  the  vocal  centers.  The  child  makes  no,  or  few,  other  asso- 
ciations with  the  ideas  gained  from  the  printed  page  before 
him.  The  words  or  ideas  which  he  reads  do  not  relate  them- 
selves in  his  mind  with  anything  else  he  has  read  or  with  other 
experiences  he  has  had,  such  that  a  complex  of  related  ideas 
are  (formed  in  his  mind  which  he  ican  reproduce  orally  or 
otherwise.  He  can  reproduce  few,  if  any,  of  the  ideas  which 
the  page  contains.  Upon  being  asked  what  he  has  read  about, 
he  remains  dumb  or  answers  merely  with  a  word  or  phrase 
contained  in  what  he  has  read.  This  type  of  reading  may  be 
suspected  from  the  monotonous  tone  with  which  it  is  delivered. 
An  extreme  example  of  this  was  that  of  a  girl  of  eleven,  found 
in  the  second  grade.  She  had  attained  the  fourth  step  in 
quantity,  and  was  very  proficient  in  her  rendering  of  phonetic 
values.  She  read  a  long  paragraph,  of  which  the  following 
is  the  beginning  sentence:  "It  was  in  the  spring  of  the  year 
1826  about  ten  o'clock,  when  Mr.  Amos  Bliss,  manager  and  one 
of  the  proprietors  of  the  Northern  Spectator,  was  in  the  garden, 
behind  his  house  planting  potatoes,"  etc.  This  selection  was 
taken  from  a  Fifth  Reader  which  she  had  never  seen.  She 
pondered  over  the  unfamiliar  words  spectator,  manager,  pro- 
prietors, and  pronounced  them  correctly,  with  very  little  loss 


STANDARDIZATION  OF  TESTS  141 

of  time.  The  other  words  in  the  selection  were  read  with 
little  or  no  hesitation.  Upon  being  asked  what  she  had  read 
about,  she  made  no  reply;  and  when  the  question  was  repeated 
she  finally  said,  "It  was  about  a  horse."  The  selection  con- 
tained no  reference  to  a  horse,  but  the  opposite  page  contained 
a  picture  of  one.  The  normal  child,  when  reading  material 
which  is  not  familiar  to  him  must  give  much  attention  to  spelling 
and  deciphering  unfamiliar  words,  he  will  often  because  of 
this  distraction,  be  unable  to  give  the  sense  of  the  selection 
read.  A  judgment  of  the  quality  of  the  child's  reading  should, 
therefore,  in  every  case  be  deduced  only  from  material  which 
he  reads  with  reasonable  facility  and  which  contains  few  if  any 
unfamiliar  words. 

2.  Appreciative. — This/type  of  reading  is  the  opposite  of  the 
mechanical  type  just  discussed.     With  this  type  there  is  usually 
expression  of  tone  in  reading  which  shows  the  child's  under- 
standing  or   appreciation   of   the   selection   read.      Upon   being 
questioned,  he  can  tell  in  a  sentence  or  more,  the  essential  ele- 
ments of  the  selection.     It  is  usually  a  sure  sign  that  the  reading 
has  been  appreciative  if  pleasure  is  shown.    However,  expression 
is  not  an  infallible  test.     Defective  children  may  be  trained  to 
read  selections  with  expression,  and  if  the  circumstances  of  the 
training   have   been   pleasant   the   child   may   incorporate   these 
pleasant   associations   into   the   reading   process   itself,    so   that 
he  seems  to  be  enjoying  the  ideas  derived  from  the  selection. 
In  such  a  case,  however,  he   fails  to  read  with  expression  or 
to  reproduce  the  sense  of  the  meaning  when  the  same  material 
is  arranged  in  unfamiliar  form. 

3.  Apperceptive.' — This  is  a  grade  of  performance  above  the 
appreciative,  in  that  there  is  a  relating  of  what  is  read  to  £ 
larger  complex  of  knowledge  or  experience  in  addition  to  the 
reproducing  of  content.     In  this  type  of  reading  the  child  can 
reproduce  orally  without  further  prompting  the  essential  details 
and   can   give   an    interpretation   of   a   selection.      Fables    Ifend 
themselves  readily  to  such  an  interpretive  test.   Defective  children 
often  can  answer  correctly  any  questions  asked  about  a  selection 
read,  but  are  unable  to  organize  it  for  themselves  and  are  unable 


142  CLARA    SCHMITT 

to  give  an  interpretation  of  its  meaning  when  the  material  is 
of  a  literary  type  other  than  that  of  didactic  narrative. 

4.  Initiative. — Reads  voluntarily.  Many  children  who  attain 
the  highest  stage  as  relates  to  quantity  in  reading  may  at  the 
same  time  really  be  able  to  gain  so  little  from  such  abstractly 
represented  ideas  that  they  never  voluntarily  read  for  their  own 
pleasure.  Many  children  who  have  not  yet  gained  the  highest 
stage  as  relates  to  quantity  still  read  voluntarily  because  of  a 
desire  to  gain  knowledge  or  to  meet  certain  social  demands. 
It  is  seldom  that  a  defective  child  reads  from  any  other  motive 
than  to  please  his  teacher. 

RESULTS  OF  READING  TESTS  FOR  NORMAL  AND  DEFECTIVE 

CHILDREN 

Two  selections  to  test  ability  in  reading  were  given  to  seven- 
teen children  of  each  grade  from  the  first  to  the  sixth,  chosen 
from  five  public  schools  of  Chicago.  Three  from  each  grade 
were  chosen  from  four  schools  and  five  from  another.  These 
schools  were  situated  in  foreign  speaking  districts.  Of  the 
eighty-five  children  tested,  thirty-eight  came  from  homes  which 
were  counted  as  English  speaking,  since  the  mother  was  able 
to  speak  English.  In  the  remaining  forty-seven  homes,  accord- 
ing to  the  testimony  of  the  children  and  the  teachers  who  knew 
them,  the  mothers  could  not  speak  English. 

The  teachers  were  given  the  following  directions  for  choosing 
the  children  for  the  test :  Select  children  who  are  average  good 
readers  for  the  grade;  do  not  select  the  very  best  reader  you 
have.  Select  them  from  that  age  of  which  you  have  the  most; 
that  is,  if  you  have  more  nine-year-old  children  than  any  other 
age  select  nine-year-old  ones.  The  first  grade  teachers  were 
asked  to  select  only  those  who  had  begun  school  in  September. 
The  teachers  consulted  the  record  of  ages  upon  entrance  in 
September.  The  tests  were  given  during  six  weeks  of  May 
and  June.  The  children  of  the  first  grade  were,  then,  near 
the  seventh  birthday;  the  second  grade  were  near  the  eighth; 
the  third  grade  were  near  the  ninth;  the  fourth  grade  were 
near  the  tenth;  and  the  fifth  grade  were  near  the  eleventh. 


STANDARDIZATION  OF  TESTS  143 

The  defective  children  who  were  given  the  tests  at  the  same 
time  and  in  the  same  way  were  between  the  ages  of  ten  and 
sixteen  who  had  been  in  the  special  rooms  for  defective  chil- 
dren for  at  least  one  year.  Many  of  them  had  been  in  these 
rooms  for  several  years.  With  one  exception  the  rooms  were 
situated  in  the  same  schools  in  which  the  normal  children  were 
tested.  There  were  five  such  rooms;  forty-six  children  of  the 
eighty  who  constituted  the  membership  of  these  rooms  fell 
within  the  conditions  chosen.  None  of  them  had  uncorrected 
defects  of  sight  or  hearing. 

The  first  of  the  selections  chosen  was  the  story  of  The  Fox 
and  the  Grapes. 

THE  FOX  AND  THE  GRAPES 

One  day  a  fox  went  down  the  road. 

"How  hungry  I  am!"  he  said.  "I  wish  I  could  find  something 
to  eat." 

Just  then  he  saw  a  grapevine.     It  had  ripe  grapes  on  it. 

"Oh,  how  good  those  grapes  look!  I  will  have  some,"  said 
the  fox. 

But  he  could  not  reach  the  grapes.  They  were  too  high  on 
the  vine. 

He  jumped  high  up  in  the  air,  but  he  could  not  get  them. 

At  last  he  went  away  hungry. 

The  birds  heard  him  say,  "Those  old  grapes  are  sour. 

They  are  not  good  for  a  fine  fox  like  me." 

But  the  birds  knew  better. 

This  selection  was  made  in  order  to  give  each  child  some- 
thing to  read  that  he  had  been  taught  in  school.  The  story  is 
one  of  the  lessons  of  the  first  reader  taught  toward  the  end 
of  the  first  year.  All  but  the  first  group  of  first  grade  children 
tested  had  read  it.  The  general  practice  of  the  school  with  such 
stories  as  this  is  to  read,  recount,  and  discuss,  and  in  some 
instances  dramatize  the  story.  If  the  children  had  not  all 
had  an  opportunity  to  recount  the  story  individually  they  had 
heard  some  of  their  classmates  do  so  and  had  joined  in  the 
discussion  of  it.  All  the  defective  children  had  had  opportunity 


144  CLARA    SCHMITT 

to  hear  it  and  read  it  and  doubtless  to  recount  it  several  times, 
since  much  attention  is  given  t(o  such  work  with  the  defective 
children.  Each  child  had  spent  at  least  two  years  in  the  first 
garde  before  entering  the  special  room. 

The  defective  children  were  all  mentally  at  least  seven  years 
of  age  according  to  the  Binet  scale.  With  the  exception  of  the 
stamp  counting  test  with  which  three  failed,  all  could  pass  all 
the  tests  for  seven  years  of  age.  All  could  do  the  Thorndike 
a  test  with  no  more  than  three  errors.  All  could  do  the  Healy- 
Fernald  Test  I  as  well  as  the  average  of  the  first  grade.  With 
tests  of  greater  complexity  there  was  much  variation. 

The  data  recorded  include  time  for  reading  the  selection, 
errors  of  pronunciation,  verbatim  reproduction  of  the  story,  and 
the  correctness  or  falseness  of  the  interpretation  of  the  motive 
of  the  fox  in  saying  the  grapes  are  sour.  This  last  item  was 
obtained  by  asking  after  the  child  had  given  his  reproduction 
of  the  story,  "Were  the  grapes  sour?"  If  the  answer  was,  "No," 
then,  "Why  did  he  say  so?"  The  interpretation  was  considered 
correct  when  the  child  indicated  that  the  fox  was  disgruntled 
at  not  being  able  to  get  the  grapes.  The  idea  was  not  always 
expressed  in  words,  but  some  times  in  an  inflection  of  the  voice 
in  the  answer,  "Just  because  he  couldn't  get  them."  If  the 
answer  to  the  first  question  was,  "Yes,"  then,  "How  did  he 
know?"  To  this  question  there  was  sometimes  an  attempt  to 
make  an  explanation  such  as  that  of  one  child,  "He  looked  at 
them,"  but  generally  there  was  silence. 

The  reproduction  was  classed  under  the  following  heads, 
scant,  adequate,  and  full.  That  reproduction  was  classed  as 
scant  which  did  not  contain  a  sufficient  number  of  the  essential 
details  to  tell  the  story,  or  which  had  them  so  mixed  or  otherwise 
wrong  that  the  story  was  not  correctly  rendered.  An  adequate 
reproduction  contained  enough  details  to  indicate  the  story,  but 
with  little  or  none  of  the  embellishing  details  of  dramatic  setting. 
The  full  reproduction  contained  all  or  nearly  all  the  items  of 
the  original  story. 

The  two  following  reproductions  were  classed  scant. 


STANDARDIZATION  OF  TESTS  145 

"The  fox  couldn't  reach  the  grapes,  he  went  away  hungry, 
the  birds  knew  better." 

"The  fox  was  hungry,  he  wanted  something  to  eat,  so  the 
birds  said  them  grapes  are  not  good,  they  are  sour." 

It  should  be  remarked  here  that  no  reproduction  was  classed 
scant  if  the  child  could  answer  a  series  of  questions  which 
would  bring  out  his  understanding  of  the  story,  such  as,  "What 
did  he  try  to  get?  What  did  he  say?"  etc. 

The  following  is  one  of  the  poorest  in  the  matter  of  detail 
of  the  reproductions  classed  as  adequate: 

"About  the  fox,  he  was  hungry,  and  he  wanted  some  grapes 
to  eat,  they  were  too  high  and  he  could  not  get  them  and  he 
said  those  grapes  are  sour." 

The  following  is  a  full  reproduction: 

"One  day  the  fox  went  down  the  road,  he  was  very  hungry, 
he  said  I  wish  I  had  something  to  eat,  then  he  saw  a  grape 
vine,  it  had  ripe  grapes  on  it,  how  nice  it  looked,  I  will  get 
some,  but  he  could  not  get  any,  then  he  went  away  hungry, 
the  birds  heard  him  say,  those  grapes  are  sour,  those  grapes  are 
not  good  for  a  fox,  but  the  birds  knew  better." 

In  recording  mispronunciations  those  words  which  the  child 
could  not  decipher  in  ten  seconds  were  classed  with  the  mis- 
pronounced. Words  mispronounced  in  reading  such  as  then 
for  they  were  called  to  the  child's  attention  with  the  question, 
"Is  it  then?"  If  he  then  pronounced  it  correctly  the  word  was 
not  classed  with  the  mispronunciations.  The  time  record  for 
normal  children  includes  time  taken  up  in  this  way.  For  the 
defective  children  a  time  record  was  seldom  of  any  significance 
because  of  the  many  corrections  and  helps  necessary  to  get  the 
child  through  the  selection. 

The  first  grade  children  of  the  first  school  tested  varied  so 
widely  from  the  other  first  grade  groups  that  their  record  could 
not  be  included  in  the  averages.  Their  performance  supported 
the  assertion  of  the  principal  that  this  particular  group  of 
foreigners  were  very  slow  in  learning  to  read.  It  is  possible 
that  their  record  would  have  been  nearer  the  average  if  they 
had  been  tested  last.  The  same  backwardness  in  reading  was 


I46  CLARA    SCHMITT 

exhibited  somewhat  by  the  second  grade  of  that  school  but  not 
sufficiently  to  make  necessary  their  elimination  from  the  av- 
erages. The  third  grade  showed  no  variation. 

Table  XLIV  shows  the  data  gained  from  Selection  I. 

TABLE  XLIV 

Data  of  Reading  Test  I 

(Reproduction  Inter- 


i\  umoer 

Average 

r 

—  \              p  relation 

of 

Average 

number 

Ade- 

/    *• 

\ 

Grade 

children 

time 

errors 

Scant 

quate 

Full 

+ 

— 

I 

12 

82" 

•5 

3 

9 

o 

6 

6 

II 

17 

62" 

o 

o 

9 

8 

6 

ii 

III 

17 

48" 

o 

o 

4 

13 

13 

4 

IV 

17 

48" 

o 

o 

5 

12 

IS 

2 

The  time  average  for  the  first  grade  of  the  above  table  had 
a  range  as  follows:  two  took  between  two  and  three  minutes 
to  read  the  selection;  six  between  one  and  two  minutes;  three 
less  than  one  minute.  The  error  average  was  made  up  of  one 
child's  five  errors  and  two  other  errors  made  by  two  children. 

The  time  average  for  the  second  grade  ranged;  one  between 
two  and  three  minutes,  four  between  one  and  two  minutes, 
seven  less  than  one  minute.  There  was  little  variation  in  the 
time  of  the  third  and  fourth  grades. 

It  is  rather  significant  of  the  small  child's  ability  to  under- 
stand the  point  of  the  fable  type  of  story  that  though  all  these 
children  had  been  taught  this  story  and  had  discussed  it  more 
or  less,  it  is  at  the  third  grade  that  it  is  understood.  The  chil- 
dren of  the  first  and  second  grades  who  gave  a  correct  inter- 
pretation probably  only  reproduced  their  teaching. 

The  errors  in  pronunciation  made  by  the  normal  children 
in  this  and  the  second  reading  test  was  always  in  favor  of  a 
word  which  had  considerable  visual  or  phonetic  resemblance 
to  the  correct  word.  The  errors  made  by  the  defective  children 
with  the  first  selection  which  was  perfectly  familiar  to  them 
in  content,  at  least,  were  absurd  so  far  as  visual  or  phonetic 
values  were  concerned,  but  were  calculated  to  fill  in  the  context. 
The  defective  child  reads,  for  instance,  that  the  fox  saw  a 
vine  with  berries  on  it.  Because  of  the  great  prevalence  of  this 
type  of  variation  the  performance  of  the  defective  group  can 


STANDARDIZATION  OF  TESTS  147 

not  be  compared  with  that  of  the  normal.  Another  type  of 
comparison  will  be  made  below. 

The  second  selection  was  chosen  because  of  its  unfamiliarity, 
of  its  wide  range  of  verbal  difficulty,  and  simplicity  of  content, 
which  at  the  same  time  possessed  a  definite  unity.  It  was  taken 
from  page  177  of  Jones'  Fifth  Reader.  This  reader  is  not 
in  use  in  the  schools,  and  probably  had  never  been  seen  by 
any  of  the  children  who  read  the  selection.  Since  the  verbal 
expression  is  rather  complex  and  the  words  used  are  not  those  of 
the  ordinary  child's  every  day  vocabulary  it  was  desirable  to  keep 
the  content  matter  simple,  that  not  too  many  difficulties  would 
confront  the  child  at  the  same  time.  The  paragraph  selected 
was :  It  ivas  a  fine  spring  morning  in  the  year  1826  about  ten 
o'clock,  when  Mr.  Amos  Bliss,  the  manager  and  one  of  the 
proprietors  of  the  Northern  Spectator,  might  Jiave  been  seen  in 
the  garden  behind  his  house  planting  potatoes.  He  heard  the 
gate  open  behind  him.,  and,  without  turning  or  looking  round, 
became  dimly  conscious  of  the  presence  of  a  boy.  But  the  boys 
of  country  villages  go  into  whosoever  garden  their  wandering 
fancy  impels  them,  and  supposing  this  boy  to  be  one  of  his  own 
neighbors,  Mr.  Bliss  continued  his  work  and  quickly  forgot  that 
he  was  not  alone. 

The  same  data  as  for  the  first  test  were  recorded,  except 
that  there  is  no  interpretation  for  this  one. 

Following  is  an  example  of  a  reproduction  classed  as 
adequate: 

"A  man  was  planting  potatoes  in  his  back  yard  and  a  boy  came 
in  and  he  thought  it  was  one  of  his  neighbor  boys  and  he  didn't 
pay  attention  to  him  and  forgot  he  wasn't  alone." 

The  following  reproduction  was  classed  as  full.  "Mr.  Bliss 
was  planting  potatoes  behind  his  house,  he  looked  up  suddenly 
and  there  was  a  boy  coming  in  his  yard,  but  in  that  country 
the  boys  go  wherever  their  fancy  impels  them  and  he  thought 
it  was  one  of  his  neighbors  and  kept  on  with  his  work  and 
after  a  while  he  forgot  that  he  was  not  alone." 

No  child  grasped  the  significance  of  the  title,  manager  and 
one  of  the  proprietors  of  the  Northern  Spectator. 


rvveragc 

iiuni'uer 

Number 

time 

errors 

Scant 

Adequate 

Full 

17 

194" 

7-8 

14 

3 

o 

17 

91" 

2.8 

13 

4 

o 

17 

74" 

I.O 

6 

7 

4 

17 

54" 

•5 

o 

9 

8 

148  CLARA    SCHMITT 

Table  XLV  shows  the  data  of  the  second  selection. 

TABLE  XLV 
Data  for  Reading  Test  II 

Average  Reproduction 

Average 
Grade 
II 
III 
IV 
V 

The  words  most  frequently  mispronounced  were,  manager 
proprietors,  Northern,  Spectator,  conscious,  whosoever,  impels, 
continued.  The  mistakes  of  the  normal  children  consist,  for 
the  most  part,  of  misplaced  accent,  the  omission  of  an  obscure 
syllable  in  long  words,  or  giving  a  different  phonetic  value  than 
is  the  right  one  for  the  word  in  which  the  letter  is  found.  Thus 
manager  becomes  manager;  proprietors  becomes  proprietors,  or 
propetors,  etc. 

RESULTS  OF  READING  TESTS  FOR  DEFECTIVE  CHILDREN 
The  reading  of  the  defective  children  presents  such  irregular 
characteristics  that  averages  which  would  present  any  meaning 
are  difficult  to  obtain.  The  children  tested  had  been  much 
drilled  in  the  story  of  the  fox  and  the  grapes.  Nevertheless 
twenty-four  of  the  forty-six  could  read  it  with  less  facility 
than  the  first  grade  children.  They  made  many  errors  of  the 
absurd  type  discussed  above.  Their  reading  consisted  of  some 
unerring  recognition  of  words  and  more  or  less  filling  in  to 
supply  a  remembered  context.  Nine  of  the  defectives  could 
give  only  a  scant  account  of  the  story  and  an  incorrect  in- 
terpretation. 

Twelve  defective  children  were  graded  as  equal  to  the  first 
grade  child  in  reading  ability.  Ten  were  graded  equal  to  the 
second  grade  child  in  ability  as  regards  the  mechanical  and 
qualitative  aspects  of  the  second  reading  test.  Two  of  the 
defectives  of  the  second  grade  could  give  an  adequate  account 
of  the  matter  read.  One  of  these  children  was  ten  years  of 
age  and  by  reason  of  this  test  and  others  was  reclassified  on 
his  record  sheet  as  only  backward  and  returned  to  the  regular 


STANDARDIZATION  OF  TESTS  149 

grades  of  the  school.  The  other,  twelve  years  of  age,  was  so 
deficient  in  other  tests  that  he  was  retained  in  the  special  room. 
Table  XLV  shows  that  it  is  only  with  the  fourth  grade  that 
such  mechanical  skill  in  reading  has  been  attained  as  to  admit 
of  sufficient  attention  to  content  to  enable  the  child  to  give  an 
adequate  reproduction  of  an  unfamiliar  selection.  With  the 
fifth  grade  such  skill  has  become  general. 

WRITING 

The  process  of  writing,  when  carried  along  with  the  accom- 
plishment of  reading,  constitutes  an  added  complexity  in  gaining 
the  symbols  of  language. 

1.  Can  form  no  letters. — This  disability  may  be  due  to  one 
or  both  of  two  things.     The  child  may  be  unable  to  analyze 
and   conceive   so  complex   a  thing  as   a  written   letter  of   the 
alphabet,  or  the  motor  control  may  be  so  poor  as  not  to  permit 
him  to  form  letters  in  the  usual  way. 

2.  Copies  words  or  sentences  legibly. 

3.  Writes  simple  sentences  from  dictation. — Many   children 
who  are  proficient  in  the  second  stage  of  the  writing  process 
cannot  write  simple  sentences  from  dictation  for  several  reasons. 
One  is  that  the  child's  memory  span  is  so  short  that  he  cannot 
remember  even  a  short  sentence  until  by  repetition  it  has  become 
very  familiar  to  him.     In  this  case  he  writes  the  first  or  second 
word  and  then  must  stop  because  he  cannot  remember  the  re- 
mainder.   Another  reason  is  that  he  may  be  unable  to  remember 
the  formation  of  the  letters,  so  that  even  though  his  memory 
may  be  of  the  type  which  can  compass  a  sentence  it  lacks  the 
ability   to   remember   the   symbols    for   recording   it.      A   third 
reason,  often   found,  is  the  child's  inability  to  learn  to  spell. 
Though  he  may  be  able  to  remember  the  sentence  and  to  write 
from  memory  all  the  letters  of  the  alphabet  he   fails  bcause 
he  cannot  remember  the  spelling  which  has  been  taught  him, 
and  he  has  no  phonetic  ability  to  enable  him  to  proceed  in- 
dependently. 

4.  Originates  sentences  to  write. — Many  defective   children 
who  are  unable  to  talk  or  discuss  the  subjects  of  their  experi- 


150  CLARA    SCHMITT 

ence  with  originality,  cannot  put  over  into  written  form  any 
sentence  not  dictated  to  them. 

5.  Can  write  a  letter  or  composition. — This  stage,  of  course, 
tests  much  more  than  the  mere  ability  to  write.  As  indicated 
in  four,  many  defective  children  can  relate  verbally  as  much 
as  a  letter  or  composition  would  contain,  and  have  mastered 
the  mechanics  of  writing,  but  they  cannot  combine  the  two 
operations.  This  is  a  test  of  the  ability  to  make  use  of  this  tool 
for  the  practical  purposes  of  social  life. 

ARITHMETIC 

The  simplest  mathematical  processes  are  the  result  of  general- 
ized abstractions.  With  arithmetical  processes  there  is  the 
necessity  for  the  use  of  symbols  to  express  such  generalizations. 
These  symbols  do  not  stand  in  one  to  one  relationship  with  a 
particular  object  or  experience.  The  symbols  of  reading  and 
writing  bear  such  a  relationship  to  the  things  they  represent. 
The  symbols  of  arithmetic  are  the  expression  of  a  conception. 
The  symbols  for  the  counting  series,  for  instance,  cannot  relate 
to  a  particular  thing  or  experience,  but  to  particularized  aspects 
of  that  abstract  quality  of  universal  relationships  which  we 
denominate  as  number.  The  simplest  process  relating  to  the 
number  relationship  is  that  of  counting.  It  is  the  first  accom- 
plishment of  the  child  in  acquiring  number  ideas. 

1.  Counts   (a)   As   a  verbal   series  merely.     Frequently   the 
very  young  child  learns  the  counting  series  before  he  learns 
that  this  series  may  be  related  to  a  series  of  objects.     Many 
defective  children  who  have  been  in  the  school  for  some  period 
of  time  know  the  counting  series  but  fail  to  relate  it  to  a  series 
of  objects.     When  asked  to  count  a  row  of  like  objects,  such 
a  child  repeats,  "i,  2,  3,"  etc.,  and  touches  the  objects  in  some 
other  order  than  that  of  the  counting  series. 

(b)   Counts  objects  such  as  lines,  beads,  etc.,  serially  arranged. 
This  is  the  second  step  in  the  learning  of  the  counting  process. 

2.  Can  make  addition  and  subtraction  combinations. — Many 
defective  children  can  count  objects,  but  this  is  as  far  as  their 
arithmetical  accomplishment  goes.     They  cannot  represent  an 


STANDARDIZATION  OF  TESTS  151 

arithmetical  situation  such  as,  "If  you  have  two  pennies  and  I 
give  you  two  more,  then  how  many  will  you  have?"  Normal 
children  of  the  age  found  in  the  first  grade,  from  six  to  seven 
years,  are  able  to  represent  such  a  situation  and  to  make  the 
combination  correctly,  though  the  attempt  is  not  made  to  teach 
them  the  process  formally.  [Chicago] 

(a)  With   objects.      The   young  child   who   has   learned   to 
count  and  who  has  not  yet  had  sufficient  experience  with  num- 
ber relationships  to  have  made  their  combinations  automatic  in 
his  memory  learns   to   make  a  concrete   representation   of   the 
situation  for  himself.     If  you  ask  him  how  much  is  five  and 
four,  he  can  represent  the  situation  with  lines  or  by  counting 
his  fingers  or  some  other  such  device.     Many  defective  children 
never  get  beyond  this  stage  in  making  number  combinations. 
They  learn  very  few  combinations,  to  such  an  extent  as  to  make 
them  automatic.     On  the  other  hand,  many  defective  children 
learn    number    combinations    as    a    mere    mechanical    memory 
process.     If  you  ask  such  a  child  how  much  is  five  and  four, 
he  answers  quickly.     However,   upon  being  asked  a  combina- 
tion which   has   not   become    automatic   with   him   he   is   quite 
mute  and  has  no  way  of  solving  his  problem. 

(b)  Can  make  combinations  only  if  put  in  the  form  of  con- 
crete ideas,  such  as,  "If  you  have  three  pennies  and  I  give  you 
two  more,  how  many  will  you  have  then?"     Many  defective 
children  will  remain  mute  if  you  ask  how  much  is  three  and 
two,  but  if  you  put  it  in  some  such  form  as  this  they  can  rep- 
resent the  situation  to  themselves  and  answer  correctly.    It  is  per- 
haps needless  to  say  that  this  type  of  defective  child  never  can 
learn  very  many  mathematical  combinations,  since  he  must  always 
depend  upon  the  imagery  of  concrete  experience  to  carry  him 
through  the  process.     With  the  normal  child  of  the  first  grade 
this  process  is  at  first  necessary,  but  he  soon  becomes  able  to 
cast  aside  this  cumbersome  method  for  (c). 

(c)  Can  make  combinations  with  symbols,  either  written  or 
oral,  unaided  by  objects  or  the  imagery  of   (b). 

(d)  Can  do  problems  involving  the  processes  of  (i)   "carry- 
ing" and   (2)   "borrowing."     Many  defective  children  who  can 


152  CLARA    SCHMITT 

add  a  long  column  cannot,  however,  accomplish  the  added  com- 
plexity of  "carrying"  when  adding  numbers  of  more  than  two 
columns.  Many  who  can  accomplish  this  feat  can  not  go  on 
and  accomplish  the  still  more  complex  process  of  "borrowing" 
in  subtraction. 

3.  Multiplication  tables. 

(a)  Knows  the  table  as  a  series  merely.     With  this  type  of 
accomplishment  the  child  learns  to  repeat  the  table,  but  if  items 
are  taken  out  of  their  order  in  the  table  he  is  unable  to  answer 
unless  he  again  begins  at  the  beginning  and  repeats  the  table 
up  to  the  desired  item.     His  learning  in  this  case  is  one  of 
mechanical  memory  of  the  type  presented  by  the  learning  of 
nonsense  syllables. 

(b)  In    the    early    stages    of    learning    the    multiplication 
table,  before  the  combinations  have  become  automatic,  the  nor- 
mal child  shows  his  appreciation  of  the  mathematical  meaning 
involved  by  counting  from  the  last  familiar  item  of  the  table 
to  gain  the  next  unfamiliar  one.    The  defective  child  who  is  not 
adept  at  mechanical  learning  learns  to  use  this  method  and  then 
continues  with  it  indefinitely. 

(c)  Knows  the  table  with  the  items  taken  at  random. 

4.  Can  do  the  processes  of  (a)  multiplication,  (b)  short  divi- 
sion, (c)  long  division. 

5.  Fractions. — The  work  with  fractions  as  it  is  given  in  the 
fifth   grade    [Chicago]    is  dependent   upon   complex  processes 
which  can  be  represented  by  symbols  only.     All  the  arithmetical 
processes  previous  to  stage  four  are  of  such  simplicity  as  to  be 
capable  of  concrete  representation  step  by  step.     The  processes 
of   addition,    subtraction,   multiplication   and    division   may   be 
learned  as  mere  mechanical  processes  which  may  have  no  rela- 
tion in  the  child's  mind  either  to  concrete  situations,  or  to  con- 
crete situations  symbolically  represented.     Many  defective  chil- 
dren learn  so  complex  a  process   as  long  division  but  never 
can  apply  it  to  the  working  out  of  concrete  problems.     It  is 
to  them  merely  a  mechanical  process  and  may  indicate  only 
an  ability  to  learn  a  process  of  such  complexity.     The  work 
with  fractions  comprises  a  set  of  processes  of  such  great  com- 


STANDARDIZATION  OF  TESTS  153 

plexity  that  it  is  hardly  possible  to  learn  them  in  the  mechanical 
way  that  long  division  can  be  learned.  It  employs  a  symbolism 
of  which  each  individual  item  represents  a  complex  concrete 
situation.  For  instance,  to  be  able  to  understand  such  a  term 
as  two-thirds  one  must  have  gone  through  the  concrete  process 
of  dividing  up  the  unit,  and  so  on  to  the  final  comprehending 
of  this  whole  process  in  the  symbol,  2/3.  One  cannot,  how- 
ever, in  the  simplest  type  of  problem  employing  the  use  of 
fi  actions,  carry  on  the  concrete  imagery  as  he  can  with  the 
simple  number  processes  discussed  above.  The  various  sets  of 
concrete  situations  represented  by  the  problem,  y2  plus  ^,  can- 
not be  kept  in  mind  to  aid  in  determining  the  result.  One  must 
carry  on  the  process  with  a  series  of  symbolically  represented 
relationships  in  which  each  symbol  comprises  the  summing  up  of 
a  complex  situation.  The  further  work  of  school  arithmetic, 
such  as  percentage,  etc.,  is  only  an  application  of  the  number 
processes  and  relationships  learned  up  to  this  point. 

6.  Practical  problems. — Many  defective  children  can  learn 
number  combinations  and  processes  up  to  the  stage  of  fractions 
but  are  unable  to  apply  these  processes  to  the  solution  of  the 
practical  problems  of  every  day  life.  The  simplest  of  such 
problems  are : 

(a.)  One-step  problems,  such  as,  "If  one  pencil  costs  two 
cents,  what  will  three  pencils  cost?"  Many  defective  children 
who  can  solve  a  problem  of  this  grade  of  complexity  cannot 
reverse  the  process  when  it  is  put,  "If  three  pencils  cost  six 
cents  what  will  be  the  cost  of  one  pencil?"  Many  who  can 
accomplish  this  feat  cannot  go  further  and  represent  to  them- 
selves and  solve  the  problem,  "If  five  pencils  cost  ten  cents 
what  will  three  pencils  cost?"  The  control  of  attention,  neces- 
sary from  the  beginning  to  the  end  of  the  problem,  and  the 
passing  over  from  one  step  to  another  to  the  final  third  step 
and  the  result  is  too  much  for  them. 

(b.)  Make  change. — Many  defective  children  can  learn  to 
make  change  only  in  so  far  as  the  conditions  of  their  lives  have 
given  them  experience.  They  may  be  able  to  make  change 
with  a  dime  or  a  quarter,  or  whatever  sum  it  may  have  been 


154  CLARA    SCHMITT 

frequently  their  fortune  to  have  to  expend,  but  are  helpless 
if  given  any  other  sum  or  combination  of  coins.  The  extent 
to  which  the  change  making  ability  may  be  learned  through 
concrete  experience  is  illustrated  by  the  case  of  a  boy  of  four- 
teen in  the  third  grade  of  the  public  school.  He  was  accustomed 
to  drive  the  carriage  which  conveyed  guests  from  the  railroad 
station  to  his  father's  small  hotel.  He  could  tell  very  quickly, 
for  instance,  what  thirteen  twenty-five  cent  fares  would  amount 
to,  but  he  could  not  do  the  arithmetical  work  of  the  third  grade 
in  the  school,  nor  could  he  do  other  types  of  making  change. 

The  normal  child  during  this  period  of  learning  money  com- 
binations uses  his  arithmetic  to  help  out  the  process  for  un- 
familiar combinations.  If  he  cannot  "mentally"  manage  such 
a  problem  as  involves  the  expenditure  of  five  cents  and  three 
cents  of  a  quarter  and  find  the  remainder,  he  applies  his  knowl- 
edge of  arithmetical  processes  to  a  solution  of  the  problem. 
The  defective  child  gets  his  method  from  oft  repeated  specific 
experience  with  such  problems. 

(c)  Tell  time. — The  children  of  the  second  grade  in  the 
public  schools  are  taught  time  units  and  their  relationship.  The 
accomplishment  of  telling  time  by  the  clock  is  one  of  too  great 
complexity  for  defective  children  of  the  normal  second  grade 
age.  Very  few  attain  it  at  all. 

RESULTS  OF  ARITHMETIC  TESTS  FOR  NORMAL  AND  DEFECTIVE 

CHILDREN 

A  series  of  tests  was  arranged  in  conformity  with  the  pre- 
ceding discussion.  The  children  who  took  the  tests  were  the  same 
group  as  took  the  reading  tests  discussed  above.  As  was  men- 
tioned they  were  chosen  for  their  ability  to  read.  It  is  possible 
that  the  arithmetic  ability  in  a  few  cases  was  not  satisfactory. 

The  tests  for  each  grade  were  arranged  to  conform  first  to 
the  requirements  of  the  curriculum  of  1912  for  the  Chicago 
schools.  In  addition  to  the  test  which  would  show  the  child's 
acquisition  of  the  required  work  for  his  grade  one  or  more 
additional  problems  were  given  to  test  his  ability  to  make  an 
independent  use  of  his  mechanical  acquisition.  It  was  attempted 


STANDARDIZATION  OF  TESTS  155 

to  use  for  this  additional  test  such  problems  as  would  either 
involve  the  next  step  to  be  made  in  the  acquisition  of  number 
conceptions,  or  would  make  such  use  of  what  had  just  been 
learned  as  was  not  specifically  taught  in  the  work  of  the  grade. 
This  attempt  was  not  entirely  successful  due  to  variations  of 
procedure  in  different  schools.  In  one  school  more  of  fractions 
was  taught  in  the  fourth  grade  than  was  required  in  the  curric- 
ulum. In  another  school  a  much  larger  experience  in  number 
work  was  given  the  first  grade  than  was  the  rule  in  the  other 
schools.  For  this  reason  the  following  series  of  tests  would 
need  to  be  given  to  a  larger  number  of  schools,  or  arranged 
with  close  reference  to  the  work  of  one  school  and  given  to  all 
the  children  in  that  school,  before  definite  statements  as  to  the 
reasoning  ability  of  normal  children  in  the  realm  of  mathematics 
could  be  made. 

In  giving  the  additional  or  reasoning  tests  the  child  was  given 
every  opportunity  to  show  his  maximum  ability  to  think.  If  his 
first  answer  was  wrong  he  was  told  so  and  asked  to  try  again 
to  think  out  his  problem.  This  was  repeated  until  it  was  apparent 
that  he  was  unable  to  think  the  problem  out  correctly.  When  a 
correct  answer  was  obtained  by  such  means  he  was  recorded  in 
the  successful  column  only  after  he  had  proved  by  his  answer 
to  other  questions  that  the  result  was  a  thoughtful  one  and  not 
a  fortunate  guess.  He  was  asked  to  tell  how  he  had  gotten 
the  answer, — the  demand  was  usually  put  in  this  form:  "How 
do  you  know  that  is  right?"  If  he  could  not  give  his  method 
clearly  enough  he  was  given  another  problem  of  the  same  kind 
with  the  numbers  changed.  Occasionally  a  child  finds  the  right 
method  for  himself  but  is  timid  about  explaining  it,  or  is  unable 
to  formulate  it. 

It  is  needless  to  say  that  no  hint  of '  the  right  method  for 
solving  a  problem  was  ever  given.  Much  encouragement  to 
the  effect  that  he  could  get  the  problem  if  he  took  plenty  of 
time  to  think  about  it  was  given.  The  child  was  always  told 
to  take  as  much  time  as  he  needed  to  think  his  problem  out  care- 
fully. Each  child  was  tested  alone  except  for  the  third,  fourth 
and  fifth  grade  written  work. 


156  CLARA    SCHMITT 

The  tests  for  the  various  grades  were  arranged  as  follows: 

Grade  I 

Required  work* — "Objects  are  counted,  using  cardinal  num- 
bers. They  are  compared  to  develop  notions  of  inequality 
and  equality.  .  .  .  All  the  work  of  the  grade  is  objective  and 
chiefly  oral."  It  is  further  recommended  that  the  work  be 
done  incidental  to  the  other  activities  of  the  grade,  drawing, 
construction  work,  etc. 

1.  (Test  for  required  work.)     Counting  a  row  of  like  objects. 

2.  (Additional.)      If  you  have  three  pennies  in  your  hand 
and  I  give  you  two  more  how  many  will  you  have  then?" 

If  the  child  hesitates  he  is  directed  to  find  out  by  counting  his 
fingers  or  by  making  marks.  He  is  given  much  encouragement 
to  do  this.  Some  children  will  say  they  do  not  know  how  to 
find  out  in  this  way,  but  if  one  keeps  on  with  the  coaxing 
encouragement  they  will  do  it.  The  small  child  is  often  diffident 
about  trying  an  untaught  or  unusual  thing. 

Grade  II 

Required  work. — "  ...  to  read  and  write  numbers  of  one  and 
two  orders ;  to  read  time  by  the  clock  to  hour,  half-hour,  quarter- 
hour;  to  answer  any  of  the  forty-five  addition  and  subtraction 
facts : 


I 

2 

3 

4 

5 

6 

7 

8 

9 

2 

3 

4 

5 

6 

7 

I 

I 

i 

i 

i 

i 

i 

i 

i 

2 

2 

2 

2 

2 

2 

8 

9 

3 

4 

5 

6 

7 

8 

9 

4 

5 

6 

7 

8 

9 

2 

2 

3 

3 

3 

3 

3 

3 

3 

4 

4 

4 

4 

4 

4 

5 

6 

7 

8 

9 

6 

7 

8 

9 

7 

8 

9 

8 

9 

9 

5 

5 

5 

5 

5 

6 

6 

6 

6 

7 

7 

7 

8 

8 

9 

...  To  make  change  within  one  dollar;  to  recognize  related 
units  of  measure,  such  as  inch,  foot;  minute,  hour,  day,  week; 
pint,  quart;  cent,  nickel,  dime,  quarter,  half-dollar,  dollar;  to  use 
the  tables  of  two's  and  three's;  to  count  by  two's  to  24  and  by 
three's  to  36 ;  to  tell  half  of  any  multiple  of  two  to  24  and  one- 
third  of  any  multiple  of  three  to  36." 

*Chicago  Public  Schools,  Course  of  Study  for  the  Elementary  Schools,  1912. 


STANDARDIZATION  OF  TESTS  157 

It  may  be  remarked  that  the  use  of  the  half-dollar  and  dollar 
are  not  attempted  by  most  second  grade  teachers  since  the  num- 
ber facts  to  be  taught  make  no  higher  combinations  than  twenty. 

1.  (Test  for  required  work.)     How  much  is  7  and  8?    How 
much  is  9  and  6?    (Additional.)     How  much  is  10  and  n  ? 

2.  (Required.)     If  you  had  10  pennies  and  spent  6  how  many 
would  be  left? 

3.  (Required.)     a.  If  you  had  a  dime  and  spent  4  cents  for 
candy  and  2  cents  for  chewing  gum,  how  much  money  would 
you  have  left?    b.  If  you  had  a  quarter  and  spent  5  cents  for 
candy  and  3  cents  for  an  apple,  how  much  moneny  would  be 
left? 

4.  (Additional.)     If  5  boys  are  in  this  room  and  3  boys  are 
in  the  other  room,  how  many  boys  would  have  to  go  from  this 
room  into  the  other  room  so  that  then  there  would  be  the  same 
number  in  each  room? 

Because  it  is  often  difficult  for  the  child  to  keep  so  long  a 
problem  in  mind  with  one  repetition,  the  problem  is  always 
repeated  immediately  as  follows.  Do  you  see  how  it  is ;  we  have 
5  boys  here  and  3  over  there,  but  we  don't  want  it  that  way,  we 
want  the  same  number  in  each  room;  how  many  would  we  have 
to  send  over  there? 

If  the  answer  to  this  problem  is  correctly  given  the  proof  of 
a  correct  process  lies  in  the  answer  to  the  question.  How  many 
will  be  in  each  room  then? 

5.  (Additional.)     a.  If  i   pencil  costs  2  cents,  what  will  4 
pencils  cost?     b.  If  5  pencils  cost  10  cents,  what  will  i  pencil 
cost?     (As  may  be  seen  by  the  table  below,  this  problem  is  not 
one  within  the  powers  of  the  second  grade  child.) 


Required  work. — Miscellaneous  problems,  involving  one  step 
only  and  making  use  of  the  units  of  measure  previously  studied- 
inch,  foot;  minute,  hour,  day,  week;  pint,  quart;  cent,  nickel, 
dime,  quarter,  half  dollar,  dollar;  pound,  dozen — and  in  addition 
the  yard,  peck  and  bushel. 

All  tables  to  and  including  twelves;  problems  involving  linear 


158  CLARA    SCHMITT 

measure;  areas  of  rectangles  found  by  drawing  and  counting, 
using  the  square  foot  and  square  yard;  fundamental  operations 
with  United  States  money,  omitting  division;  reading  and  writ- 
ing numbers  including  five  orders ;  fundamental  operations,  mul- 
tiplier or  divisor  not  to  exceed  two  figures. 

1.  (Test   for  required  work.)      Multiplication   Table.      Care 
was  taken  to  determine  whether  the  table  was  rendered  as  a 
feat  of  mechanical  memory  more  or  less  perfect,  as  is  sometimes 
the  case  with  defective  children,  or  whether  the  child  had  a  cor- 
rect mathematical  conception  of  the  table.    The  child  who  under- 
stands what  the  table  means,  when  he  comes  to  an  unfamiliar 
item  of  the  table  knows  that  he  can  count  from  the  last  familiar 
item  to  gain  the  required  unfamiliar  one. 

2.  (Test  for  required  work.)     Fundamental  operations: 


a.     2813  b.     3421  c.     12)36281 

—1482  X  26 


3.  If  you  had  a  dollar  and  spent  47  cents  how  much  money 
would  be  left?     This  problem  was  used  to  see  if  when  in  any 
case  the  problem  was  too  difficult  a  one  to  do  "mentally"   a 
higher  process  than  the  one  resorted  to  by  the  second  grade  in 
such  situations  was  used.     The  second  grade  child  could  only 
make  marks  to  find  the  answer  to  an  unfamiliar  combination. 
The  third  grade  child  has  the  advantage  of  the  process  of  sub- 
traction with  "borrowing." 

4.  (Additional.)     If  five  pencils  cost  ten  cents,  what  will  three 
pencils  cost? 

5.  (Additional.)     a.  If  you  had  twelve  cents  and  lost  half 
of  your  money,  how  much  would  be  left?    b.  If  you  had  fifteen 
cents  to  divide  equally  among  three  boys  how  much  would  each 
one  get? 

Grade  IV 

Required  work.  Area  of  rectangles,  dimensions  limited  to 
like  integral  units. 

Time,  including  the  number  of  days  in  each  month;  methods  of 
proving  the  'fundamental  operations,  terminology  used  in  funda- 


STANDARDIZATION  OF  TESTS  159 

mental  operations;  reading  and  writing  numbers  of  not  more 
than  two  periods;  problems,  introducing  bills,  involving  the 
common  measures  previously  studied  and  using  incidentally  the 
half,  third,  fourth,  sixth,  eighth,  ninth,  twelfth,  and  sixteenth, 
involving  no  remainders ;  areas  of  irregular  plane  surfaces  which 
may  be  divided  easily  into  rectangles;  perimeters  of  rectilinear 
plane  figures ;  mile  and  rod ;  multiplication :  multiplier,  and  any 
two-  or  three-digit  number;  proofs;  division:  divisor,  any  two- 
or  three-digit  number;  proofs. 

1.  (Test  for  required  work.)     48)64911. 

2.  (a)  y2oii2  =        (b)  y3  of  12  —        (c)  14  of  12  = 
(d)  y6  of  12  = 

3.  (Additional.)     a.  Which  is  larger  a  half  of  something  or 
a  third  of  the  same  thing?    b.  A  third  of  something  or  a  fourth 
of  the  same  thing,     c.  A  fifth  of  something  or  a  tenth  of  the 
same  thing? 

In  general  a  correct  answer  is  given  :for  a,  but  frequently  a 
wrong  one  for  b.  The  problem  may  then  be  put  as  follows : 
which  way  would  you  get  the  largest  piece,  if  you  were  one 
of  the  three  boys  who  divided  a  pie  among  themselves  or  if 
you  were  one  of  four  boys  ?  The  normal  child  gives  the  correct 
answer.  Then :  If  you  were  one  of  three  boys  what  part  would 
your  piece  be?  If  you  were  one  of  four  boys?  Which,  then, 
is  the  larger,  a  third  or  a  fourth?  The  normal  child  quickly 
sees  the  principle  of  such  problems  and  answers  similar  follow- 
ing ones  correctly.  The  defective  child,  though  he  may  be  brought 
to  decide  correctly  over  and  over  again  to  which  group  he  should 
belong  to  get  the  larger  piece,  makes  the  same  type  of  error  with 
the  next  similar  problem  abstractly  presented. 

4.  (Additional.)     If  you  had  twelve  cents  and  lost  two-thirds 
of  your  money,  how  much  would  you  then  have?     This  prob- 
lem contains  two  new  things;   a   consideration  of  two-thirds, 
and  the  use  of  it  as  a  quantity  in  a  problem.     If  the  child  hesi- 
tated long  or  seemed  nonplussed  by  the  complexity  of  his  prob- 
lem he  was  asked,  How  much  is  two-thirds  of  twelve?     If  a 
correct  answer  was  obtained,  then  the   further  encouragement 
was  given  in  this  form:     Then  tell  me,  if  you  lose  two-thirds 
of  your  twelve  cents  how  much  is  left?     Of  nine  fourth-grade 


160  CLARA   SCHM1TT 

children  who  passed  this  test,  four  needed  such  encourage- 
ment. 

5.  (Additional.)  You  had  some  money  and  lost  two-thirds  of 
it  and  then  there  was  eight  cents  left,  how  much  did  you  have 
at  first? 

This  problem,  as  may  be  seen  by  Table  XLVI,  is  too  diffi- 
cult for  the  fourth  grade. 

Grade  V 

First  half-year  (semester)  of  the  grade.  Fractions. —  Con- 
cretely and  orally;  fractional  equivalents;  sum  or  difference  of 
any  two  fractions  within  the  limits  of  halves  and  sixteenths. 

Second  half-year  (semester)  of  the  grade.  Fractions. — Addi- 
tion, subtraction,  multiplication,  division,  comparison,  and  ratio 
of  fractions.  Suggestion:  fractions  arranged  in  as  many  dif- 
ferent pairs  as  possible :  ^  +  ^6;^  —  H '>  Y2  ~+~  H '>  J/2  com- 
pared with  H( ratio) ;  H,  compared  with  J^;  J4  of  Y&;  }i  of  #. 

1.  (Test  for  required  work.)     %  -\-  34  = 

2.  (Additional.)     If  four  dozen  apples  cost  $1.50,  what  will 
three  apples  cost? 

Table  XLVI  shows  the  number  of  children  of  each  grade 
who  succeeded  with  the  tests  as  far  as  each  individual  was  able 
to  go.  Each  child  was  given  all  the  problems  included  in  higher 
grades  with  which  there  was  a  possibility  of  success.  Each  grade 
was  given  such  problems  of  lower  grades  as  were  not  implicit 
in  the  work  of  the  grade  being  tested. 

Examination  of  the  table  shows  that  with  each  grade  success 
was  almost  universal  with  the  required  work  of  the  grade.  The 
numbers  in  bold  type  at  the  head  of  each  column  indicate  the 
problems  testing  required  work.  The  few  failures  in  required 
work  may  have  been  due  to  the  fact  that  the  children  were  chosen 
for  satisfactory  reading  ability.  The  results  of  the  tests  with 
defective  children  are  given  first  in  the  absolute  numbers  and  in 
the  line  below  reduced  to  a  scale  of  seventeen.  The  table  shows 
that  about  two-thirds  of  the  defective  children  were  able  to 
accomplish  the  required  work  of  the  second  grade ;  one-third  had 
learned  the  multiplication  table  and  one-sixth  had  learned  to 
multiply.  The  success  of  the  defective  children  with  the  addi- 


H 

x| 


C3 


1 

01 

o 

^ 

la 

^ 
•H 

M 

i 

L> 

- 

'O 

g 

« 

J 

1 
1 

i 

.» 

3 
3 

H 

H 

J 

10 

*T^ 
n3 

O 

-o 

l_i 

CO 

, 

•4 

H 

* 

J 
3 

Tj 

O 

-o 

G 
8 

CO 

t 

u 
0 

« 

rt 

0 

M 

en 

E 

Number 

•o 

rt 

O 

CO  M        VD   <*5 
rt  J3         rt  JO 


t^  Tf  JX  IT) 

rt  .0        rt  Xi 


>  > 


rt  ^Q    V  *O 


OMO 

rt  ,0 


00 
w   ID 


vq 

VD 


«S"P    OT 

s 


162  CLARA    SCHMITT 

tional  work  of  the  grades  where  it  is  indicated  does  not  mean 
that  the  mental  process  of  such  individuals  was  equivalent  to  that 
of  the  children  belonging  to  those  grades.  An  uncritical  cross 
section  type  of  test  may  lead  one  to  infer  that  such  is  the  case. 
It  is  in  watching  the  defective  child  in  the  schoolroom  that  one 
comes  to  realize  that  he  does  not,  for  instance,  originate  the  pro- 
cess of  counting  marks  to  gain  new  number  combinations  but  the 
process  must  be  drilled  into  his  mind  with  more  or  less  repetition. 

The  youngest  of  the  defective  children  had  had  four  times  as 
much  school  experience  as  the  first  grade  children,  and  twice  as 
much  as  the  second  grade  children.  It  is  probable  that  the 
youngest  of  these  children  and  certainly  the  older  ones,  had 
had  so  much  experience  with  the  money  problems  of  the  second 
grade,  for  instance,  as  to  make  the  result  in  effect  that  of  drill. 

Reference  to  the  table  shows  that  problem  5b  under  the  second 
grade  belongs  to  third  grade  abilities.  The  second  grade  child 
has  no  knowledge  of  division.  Many  second  grade  children  at- 
tempted to  solve  the  problem  by  an  arrangement  of  marks  but 
became  confused  in  the  process  and  gave  up. 

Methods  of  Solving  Problems. — The  first  grade  child  finds 
out  combinations  under  ten  by  counting  his  fingers.  A  few  can 
be  induced  to  make  marks  on  paper.  This  grade,  however,  uses 
pencil  but  little  and  this  probably  accounts  for  the  child's  dis- 
inclination to  use  this  method.  Care  was  always  observed  in 
the  examiner's  encouragement  of  the  child  to  use  the  pencil  to 
stop  short  of  actual  showing  this  new  method  of  representing  his 
number  situation.  The  first  grade  child  fails  with  the  finger 
method  for  combinations  above  ten  since  he  has  no  more  fingers 
and  must  use  some  a  second  time  in  the  process  of  gaining  such  a 
combination.  Though  he  attempts  to  do  so  the  process  is  too 
complex  and  he  fails  to  get  a  correct  result.  Older  children  who 
must  resort  to  counting  to  find  large  combinations  can  use  the 
finger  method  successfully.  The  first  grade  child  fails  in  sub- 
traction of  numbers  above  five  because  of  the  difficulty  he  finds 
in  using  the  forefinger  of  one  hand  both  to  count  and  to  represent 
one  of  the  items  of  his  problem. 

Of  the  second  grade  children  nine  could  give  the  combinations 
of  the  first  problem  automatically,  that  is  they  did  not  need 


STANDARDIZATION  OF  TESTS  163 

to  count.  The  others  counted  either  fingers,  marks,  or  silently. 
In  the  method  of  silent  counting  the  child  did  not  have  the  com- 
bination so  thoroughly  learned  as  to  do  away  with  counting 
altogether,  but  he  could  represent  the  situation  "mentally,"  by 
means  oi  some  type  of  imagery  not  reduced  to  concrete  means 
such  as  marks. 

Of  the  defective  children  five  could  make  such  number  com- 
binations automatically. 

The  second  grade  child  does  the  problem  of  3a  and  3b  with 
the  use  of  marks.  The  failures  with  3b  were  due  to  the  neces- 
sary complexity  of  many  marks,  roblem  5a  was  solved  by  count- 
ing by  2's  or  by  making  marks  of  four  groups  of  two  each. 

Each  second  grade  child  was  given  problem  3  of  the 
third  grade  group.  Each  child  began  industriously  to  make  a 
hundred  marks,  but  because  of  the  length  of  time  necessary 
for  such  a  procedure  was  not  permitted  to  finish  the  problem. 

The  third  grade  children  had  for  the  most  part  made  the 
multiplication  tables  of  4's  and  7*3,  the  tables  used  for  the  test, 
automatic  with  only  an  occasional  stop  to  count  up  for  an  un- 
familiar item.  Of  the  defective  children  six  had  gained  so 
much  facility  with  the  tables.  Twelve  defective  children  could 
recite  the  tables  correctly  by  counting  up  from  each  last  item. 
Some  of  the  defective  children  had  a  more  or  less  complete 
mechanical  acquaintance  with  the  tables,  but  when  memory  failed 
they  broke  down  with  no  method  of  finding  the  unfamiliar  item. 

The  third  grade  child  solves  problem  3  either  "mentally" 
or  by  means  of  subtraction.  The  failures  of  the  two  third 
grade  children  were  due  to  the  complexity  of  the  double 
"borrowing." 

Problem  4  is  too  difficult  a  one  for  the  third  grade,  but  is 
more  nearly  suited  to  fourth  grade  abilities. 

Of  the  fourth  grade  children  the  five  of  the  first  school 
tested  had  had  little  of  the  required  work  with  fractions.  Their 
experience  had  been  only  with  the  fraction  one-half.  The  three 
failures  with  2a,  2b,  and  2c  were  in  this  group,  and  consequently 
the  three  failures  with  problem  three;  all  of  the  five  failed  with 
problem  four.  Problem  five  is  too  difficult  for  the  fourth  grade, 
but  is  suitable  to  fifth  grade  abilities. 


XI 

FACTORS  INVOLVED  IN  THE  MENTAL  CLASSIFICA- 
TION OF  CLINIC  CASES 

Throughout  this  study  the  reaction  of  feeble-minded  children 
seen  at  the  clinic  has  been  compared  with  that  of  the  normal 
children  of  the  schools.  There  does  not  exist  any  fixed  accepted 
standard,  except  the  arbitrary  one  laid  down  by  the  Binet  tests, 
for  distinguishing  the  feeble-minded  from  the  mentally  normal. 
There  come  to  any  general  clinic  many  different  types  of  cases 
of  social  deviation.  In  the  Juvenile  Court  Clinic  the  cases  are  of 
moral  deviation;  in  the  public  school  clinic  the  cases  are 
those  which  show  unsatisfactory  reaction  to  the  school  situa- 
tion, of  conduct  or  of  progress  in  mastering  the  subject-matter 
of  the  curriculum.  It  is  the  function  of  the  clinic  to  determine 
from  what  cause  this  social  deviation  springs.  The  causes 
may  be  in  the  mentality,  the  environment,  the  physical  condition, 
or  some  peculiarity  of  individual  interest  or  temperament.  One 
or  all  of  these  may  combine  to  produce  a  deviation  which  brings 
the  child  into  the  clinic  for  classification  and  advice. 

The  first  duty  of  the  psychological  clinic  is,  naturally,  to 
separate  the  normal  in  innate  mental  ability  from  the  defective. 
The  lower  grades  of  feeble-mindedness  are  apparent  to  every 
one.  The  defective  reaction  of  the  imbecile  to  almost  every  con- 
ceivable situation  is  so  marked  as  never  to  be  overlooked,  at 
least  after  the  child  is  five  or  six  years  of  age.  There  is,  how- 
ever, a  group  at  the  upper  end  of  the  moron  class  which  is  not 
so  easily  detected. 

This  type  is  perceptually  bright;  can  reproduce  past  experi- 
ences in  conversation  and  so  give  a  superficial  impression  of 
brightness;  is  sometimes  even  loquacious;  may  be  given  to 
bragging  a  bit  and  so  give  the  impression  of  possessing  those 
powers  of  imagination  from  which  high  standards  of  action 
are  derived;  are  ready  in  promises  of  good  behavior;  can  orien- 
tate themselves  in  a  large  city  and  take  proper  physical  care 


STANDARDIZATION  OF  TESTS  165 

of  themselves  among  its  dangers;  if  working,  they  can  often 
perform  satisfactorily,  certain  types  of  routine  work  sufficiently 
well  to  make  them  self-supporting.  How  to  separate  this  class 
from  that  large  class  of  children  who  are  not  defective  but 
backward,  pedagogically  considered,  frequently  becomes  a  diffi- 
cult task  for  the  clinic  psychologist.  Holmes  (21)  discusses 
this  problem  under  the  title  o«f  "Curable  and  Incurable  Backward- 
ness." He  says:  "If  a  child  is  curably  backward,  he,  by  that 
fact  alone,  enters  into  a  great  class  of  children  retarded  from 
any  cause  whatsoever;  if  he  is  incurably  backward,  he  enters 
into  another  great  class  commonly  called  feeble-minded  or 
mentally  defective.  Such  a  distinction  is  fraught  with  the 
gravest  practical  importance  for  the  child  and  all  concerned' 
with  him.  The  determination  of  this  vital  step  is  one  of  the 
most  important  in  making  a  diagnosis.  Sufficient  is  it  now  to 
say  that  the  distinction  does  not  rest  upon  any  symptom-com- 
plex or  appearance  of  the  child  alone.  Curably  backward  and 
incurably  backward  children  often  look  exactly  alike;  know 
about  the  same  amount  of  school  lore ;  act  about  alike  in  society, 
and  sometimes  even, — if  there  is  any  advantage  either  way, 
the  incurably  backward  or  feeble-minded  child  has  it." 

Holmes'  method  of  determination  mainly  rests  upon  the  de- 
velopmental history  of  the  child  in  connection  with  his  present 
physical  condition.  If  he  finds  in  the  past  history  nothing  to 
lead  one  to  suspect  that  the  child  has  suffered  a  brain  lesion, 
and  if  the  present  physical  condition  is  one  which  needs  correc- 
tion, he  is  classed  as  curably  backward. 

This  basis  of  classification,  however,  removes  the  case  from 
the  field  of  psychology  and  places  it  in  the  field  of  medicine. 
The  child's  developmental  history  is  certainly  of  very  great  im- 
portance for  the  clinical  psychologist,  but  no  such  history,  how- 
ever full  it  may  be  of  suggestions  of  brain  lesion,  can  establish 
the  extent  of  such  lesions  or  their  effect  upon  mental  function- 
ing. It  is  in  the  province  of  the  psychologist  to  so  investigate 
and  analyze  the  child's  mental  complex  as  to  show  the  quality  and 
extent  of  his  mental  defectiveness  if  such  exists.  The  line,  or 
as  Tredgold  terms  it,  "the  gulf"  which  divides  the  highest 


166  CLARA    SCHMITT 

mental  defective  from  the  normal  has  been  indicated  by  various 
writers  who  have  become  acquainted  with  this  class  where  its 
individuals  have  gained  entrance  into  institutions  for  the  feeble- 
minded. The  standardizations  of  various  authors  have  been 
brought  together  by  Holmes  (21 )  : 

By  Goddard  (15).     Moron: 

High-grade:  can  do  fairly  complicated  work  with  only 
occasional  or  no  supervision;  can  run  simple  machinery,  take 
care  of  animals;  only  unable  to  plan. 

By  Barr  (22).    Imbecile: 

High-grade:   trainable  in  manual  and  intellectual  arts. 

By  Binet  (23).     Feeble-minded: 

Every  child  is  feeble-minded  who  knows  how  to  communicate 
with  his  fellows  by  word  and  by  writing  but  who  exhibits  a 
retardation  of  two  or  three  years,  in  the  course  of  his  studies, 
unless  that  retardation  should  be  on  account  of  insufficient 
training. 

By  Tredgold  (24).    Feeble-minded: 

First-grade:  can  make  tolerable  progress  in  elementary 
school  work;  can  write  a  simple  letter,  read  children's  books, 
can  perform  simple  arithmetical  exercises  mentally.  Can  do 
good  manual  work. 

In  the  above  definitions  Goddard  and  Tredgold  evidently  have 
in  mind  a  qualitative  distinction  between  the  high-grade  defective 
and  the  normal.  Barr  and  Binet  indicate  that  the  distinction 
may  be  quantitative.  Goddard  emphasizes  the  qualitative  distinc- 
tion in  his  last  clause,  "only  unable  to  plan."  It  is  this  distinction 
which  the  writer  uses  as  the  dividing  line  between  the  normal 
and  the  defective.  In  the  foregoing  discussion  of  tests  where 
the  types  of  reaction  may  be  distinguished  as  those  of  planned, 
trial  and  error,  and  chance  the  last  two  have  always  been 
found  to  be  characteristic  o»f  the  feeble-minded  class  and  cases  are 
classified  as  such  if  their  invariable  reaction  to  tests  is  below 
that  of  the  planned  or  consciously  controlled  reaction  type  and 
when  the  child's  history  is  in  conformity  with  the  mental  plane 
the  psychological  tests  determine  for  him. 


STANDARDIZATION  OF  TESTS  167 

The  histories  of  these  individuals  show  them  quite  lacking  in 
the  ability  to  plan  their  conduct  in  conformity  with  the  complex 
requirements  of  civilized  life  in  such  a  way  as  to  make  them  really 
a  part  of  the  social  class  in  which  they  may  be  found.  The  child's 
reaction  to  the  home  situation,  the  school  situation,  and  his 
type  of  moral  delinquency  show,  as  well  as  the  mental  tests, 
the  inability  of  this  type  of  child  to  plan  or  reason.  The  parents 
of  such  children  complain  of  a  lack  of  responsibility  of  the 
child  in  the  home  life.  He  cannot  be  trusted  as  the  other  chil- 
dren to  go  on  errands  or  to  conduct  himself  properly  in  other 
ways.  The  teacher  says  that  he  does  not  learn  and  makes  poor 
progress  in  the  school.  When  his  acquirements  of  school  lore 
are  examined,  it  is  found  that  he  has  been  capable  of  learning 
more  or  less  in  a  mechanical  way;  he  makes  some  degree  of 
progress  in  reading  and  arithmetic  but  the  working  over  of  any 
learned  content  to  fit  the  needs  of  a  new  situation  is  beyond 
his  ability. 

The  arithmetic  gained  by  the  child  who  may  be  classed  as 
mentally  defective  is  merely  that  of  a  mechanical  nature.  Such 
applications  as  he  can  make  of  his  knowledge  is  small  in  extent 
and  usually  the  result  of  oft  repeated  bits  of  specific  experience. 

However  much  he  may  be  able  to  learn  to  recognize  printed 
words,  he  is  not  able  to  use  reading  to  an  appreciable  extent 
in  gaining  organized  bodies  of  knowledge.  The  use  of  reading 
and  arithmetic  tests  and  other  tests  involving  the  use  of  symbols 
can  be  made  to  show  the  extent  to  which  he  is  lacking  in  the 
ability  to  use  abstract  or  symbolic  materials  of  thought.  The 
use  of  reading,  writing,  and  arithmetic  as  tests  of  mental  ability 
are  discussed  above. 

That  part  of  the  child's  history  which  relates  to  the  school 
must  take  into  consideration  grade  with  reference  to  age  and 
attainments  with  reference  to  the  curriculum.  One  finds  many 
large  defective  children  in  grades  a  year  or  more  behind  that 
which  corresponds  with  the  chronological  age,  but  at  the  same 
time  far  in  advance  of  their  attainments  in  school  subjects. 

The  high  grade  defective  child  often  becomes  a  dilinquent 
of  a  more  or  less  serious  character  in  the  neighborhood,  because 


168  CLARA    SCHMITT 

he  is  not  able  to  apply  to  his  own  conduct  such  formal  rules  of 
conduct  concerning  the  rights  of  others,  as  he  may  have  been 
taught  in  the  school  or  other  social  agencies. 

The  type  of  delinquency  or  other  social  reaction  shows,  fre- 
quently, a  correlation  with  mental  ability  as  determined  by  other 
tests.  The  defective  delinquent  is  usually  the  follower  of  a  more 
capable  companion  who  plans  the  escapades  in  which  they  en- 
gage. The  delinquent  is  a  tool  or  a  dupe  in  the  plan  of  others. 
The  defective's  type  of  delinquency  is  simple  so  far  as  its  mental 
content  is  concerned,  however  serious  it  may  be  in  its  social 
or  economic  aspects.  The  fourteen-year-old  boy  who  robbed 
his  mother  of  a  sum  of  hoarded  money,  and  then  tied  himself 
and  did  other  things  to  simulate  an  attack  from  the  outside 
was  not  a  mental  defective.  The  twelve-year-old  boy  who  or- 
ganized and  led  a  gang  of  older  boys  in  a  robbery  of  a  large 
jewelry  store,  escaping  successfully  with  the  booty  was  not  a 
defective.  The  fourteen-year-old  boy  who  on  several  occasions 
went  into  unguarded  houses  and  wantonly  destroyed  things, 
repeating  the  offense,  though  each  time  he  was  apprehended 
and  brought  before  the  Juvenile  Court  was  a  defective.  The 
boy  who  at  fourteen  years  of  age  had  appeared  before  the  court 
fourteen  times  for  robbery,  but  who  each  time  carried  out  the 
plan  of  some  other  boy  who  escaped  upon  the  approach  of 
the  police  was  a  defective;  as  was  the  boy  of  twelve  who  en- 
gaged in  a  robbery  of  large  extent  with  some  others  and  selected 
as  his  part  of  the  booty  all  the  pennies  of  the  collection,  leaving 
to  the  others  the  bills  of  large  denomination. 

Such  popular  types  of  evidence  discussed  above  can  be  ac- 
cepted of  course  only  as  indicative  of  a  possible  mental  status. 
They  must  be  used  with  judgment.  The  parent  may  place  his 
expectations  of  responsibility  too  high;  the  child's  progress  in 
the  school  may  be  influenced  by  other  factors  than  that  of 
mental  ability;  and  many  children  in  whom  there  is  no  hint 
of  mental  defectiveness  become  delinquent.  Formal  tests  of 
mental  ability  must  exercise  their  function  either  of  corroborating 
or  disputing  the  social  judgment. 

A   formal  test  of  mental  ability  on  the  other  hand  must 


STANDARDIZATION  OF  TESTS  169 

always  be  carefully  evaluated  with  reference  to  the  situation 
in  which  the  child  is  found.  Such  care  is  especially  demanded 
under  the  conditions  of  a  clinic  for  delinquents.  The  examiner 
must  also  be  careful  to  distinguish  a  type  of  reaction  due  to 
temperament  from  that  due  to  defective  mental  ability.  The 
exceedingly  shy  child  or  the  child  who  lacks  confidence  in  him- 
self may  react  to  tests  in  a  different  way  than  his  mental  ability, 
uninfluenced  by  such  temperament,  might  permit.  An  emotional 
upheaval  such  as  a  delinquent  may  suffer  is  sometimes  suc- 
ceeded by  such  mental  apathy  as  prevents  normal  reaction  to 
the  tests  which  require  judgment  or  a  high  degree  of  control 
of  attention.  One  such  case  was  that  of  a  boy  of  sixteen  who 
had  graduated  from  the  eighth  grade  with  honors,  had  done 
the  work  satisfactorily  of  a  trusted  errand  boy  in  a  large  bank, 
was  a  reader  of  classical  types  of  literature  and  had  organized 
bodies  of  historical  and  geographical  data  gained  from  his 
reading,  but  failed  in  nearly  all  formal  mental  tests  given  him. 
The  classification  of  this  boy's  mental  ability  could  not  be  based 
on  his  reactions  to  formal  tests. 

One  must  also  be  able  to  distinguish  unfavorable  reactions 
due  to  the  deteriorations  or  disturbances  caused  by  certain  men- 
tal or  nervous  diseases  from  those  due  to  mental  subnormality  or 
defectiveness. 

Thus  school  and  social  history  and  the  evidence  of  formal  tests 
determine  whether  or  not  the  child  is  normal  or  below  in  mental 
ability.  If  his  reactions  go  to  show  that  his  ability  to  learn 
is  only  mechanical;  that  he  is  not  capable  of  making  new  appli- 
cations of  the  content  of  previous  learning,  he  is  classed  as 
mentally  defective,  or,  as  Holmes  terms  it,  "incurably  back- 
ward." If  he  is  classed  as  normal  in  mental  ability,  then,  the 
cause  of  his  defective  reactions  to  the  school  and  social  situa- 
tion must  be  sought  in  physical  conditions,  social  environment, 
individual  peculiarity  of  temperament  or  interest  or  mental  com- 
plex. Each  one  of  these  possibilities  opens  a  new  realm  for 
psychological  investigation. 

This,  then,  is  the  proposed  criterion  for  discriminating  the 
normal  from  the  subnormal  or  mentally  defective.  In  order  to 


i/o  CLARA   SCHMITT 

establish  valid  standards  in  the  classification  of  individual  cases 
th  physician  and  the  psychologist  must  cooperate.  We  need  more 
knowledge  than  is  now  available  of  the  mental  effects  of  certain 
curable  and  incurable  physical  conditions  and  defects.  Such 
problems  as  the  mental  effects  of  malnutrition,  or  the  effect 
upon  certain  learning  complexes  of  defective  vision  and  hearing 
require  careful  observation  and  correlation  over  long  periods  of 
time  of  the  mental  and  physical  functionings  for  their  solution. 
Until  much  more  of  such  knowledge  is  available  many  individ- 
uals can  be  only  tentatively  classed  as  curably  or  incurably 
backward. 

This  basis  for  the  determination  of  the  mentally  defective 
is  in  conformity  with  the  view  of  Stern  (25)  in  his  discussion 
of  "The  Nature  of  Intelligence."  He  says : 

"Naturally,  we  cannot  begin  our  work  without  a  preliminary 
definition  of  intelligence,  however  provisional  it  may  be.  And 
this  definition  must  be  neither  too  broad  nor  too  narrow. 

"Many  psychiatrists  have  used  a  definition  of  intelligence 
that  is  too  broad.  They  use  intelligence,  in  fact,  to  include 
mental  attainments  of  all  kinds,  all  those  mental  qualities,  then, 
that  are  not  volitional  or  emotional.  If  this  position  be  taken, 
it  follows,  evidently  that  the  examination  of  immediate  memory, 
of  ability  to  learn,  of  range  of  information,  of  fidelity  of  re- 
port, or  of  discriminative  sensitivity  is  just  as  much  a  con- 
stituent part  of  intelligence  as  the  examination  of  ability  to 
apprehend,  to  synthetize,  of  capacity  to  judge,  to  conclude,  to 
define,  to  criticize,  etc.  Again,  a  question  that  is  very  important 
for  us,  viz. :  to  what  extent  intelligence  really  enters  into  these 
first-named  activities,  and  whether  and  in  what  way  it  shows 
signs  of  its  presence  in  them,  becomes  absurd.  But  the  ad- 
vance made  in  the  recent  development  of  intelligence  testing, 
in  contrast  to  the  uncritical  determination  of  mental  level  by 
any  sort  of  questions  and  tests,  consists  in  the  fact  that  we 
not  only  limit  intelligence  by  setting  it  over  against  the  emotive 
and  volitional  nature  of  an  individual,  but  also  ascribe  to  it  a 
definitely  restricted  place  within  the  mental  ifunctions. 

"This  delimitation  of  the  sphere  of  intelligence  that  is  even 


STANDARDIZATION  OF  TESTS  171 

now  essential  cannot  be  affected,  in  my  opinion,  from  a  phe- 
nomenological,  but  only  from  a  teleological  point  of  view.  In 
fact,  my  definition  is  this: 

"Intelligence  is  a  general  capacity  of  an  individual  consciously 
to  adjust  his  thinking  to  new  requirements :  it  is  general  mental 
adaptability  to  new  problems  and  conditions  of  life. 

"This  definition  differentiates  intelligence  clearly  from  other 
mental  capacities. 

"The  fact  that  the  adjustment  is  made  to  the  new  distinguishes 
intelligence  from  memory  whose  fundamental  teleological  fea- 
ture is  the  conservation  and  utilization  of  conscious  contents 
already  given. 

"The  fact  of  adaptation,  again,  emphasizes  the  dependence 
of  the  performance  upon  external  factors,  on  the  problems  and 
demands  of  life,  and  thus  distinguishes  intelligence  from  genius, 
whose  nature  is  to  create  the  new  spontaneously. 

"Finally,  the  'fact  that  the  capacity  is  a  general  capacity 
distinguishes  intelligence  from  talent  the  characteristic  of  which 
is  precisely  the  limitation  of  efficiency  to  one  kind  of  content. 
He  is  intelligent,  on  the  contrary,  who  is  able  easily  to  effect 
mental  adaptation  to  new  requirements  under  the  most  varied 
conditions  and  in  the  most  varied  fields.  If  talent  be  a  material 
efficiency,  intelligence  is  a  formal  efficiency. 

"I  trust  that  these  distinctions  may  serve  to  lessen  the  con- 
fusion that  has  been  current.  It  is  not  so  long  ago,  indeed, 
that  in  psychiatry  'information  tests'  were  carried  on  as  'in- 
telligence tests,'  thereby  confusing  memory  and  intelligence. 
And  we  often,  even  nowadays,  find  intelligence,  and  talent  con- 
fused in  everyday  life.  In  school,  for  instance,  a  teacher  of  a 
special  subject  like  mathematics,  who  perceives  the  special  gift 
of  a  pupil  in  that  field,  may  easily  come  to  believe  without  further 
evidence  that  this  pupil  has  general  ability,  or  in  other  words, 
to  rate  him  as  an  intelligent  pupil. 

But  we  should  not  interpret  this  delimitation  to  mean  the 
erection  of  sharply  distinct  faculties,  as  in  the  old  faculty  theory. 
Intelligence,  for  instance,  does  not  function  by  itself  and  memory 
by  itself;  rather,  every  operation  of  memory  is  more  or  less 


172  CLARA    SCHMITT 

impregnated  with  intellectual  functions  and  vice  versa:  the  ex- 
tent of  this  interconnection  can  be  indicated  only  by  the  correla- 
tion of  the  tested  symptoms.  But  just  on  account  of  this  com- 
posite character  of  every  actual  mental  process  it  seems  to  me 
that  the  definition  of  intelligence  I  have  given  above  is  indis- 
pensible  as  a  regulative  principle  for  further  investigation;  I 
mean  that  any  sort  of  perceptive,  memorial  or  attentive  activity 
is  at  the  same  time  an  intelligent  activity  just  in  so  far  as 
it  includes  a  new  adjustment  to  new  demands." 

With  this  principle  for  guidance  a  system  of  mental  grading 
independent  of  age  may  be  constructed.  A  child  of  any  age 
may  be  on  the  same  mental  level  as  a  child  of  any  other  age, 
though  their  acquisitions  of  knowledge  due  to  different  types 
of  experience  and  training  may  be  quite  dissimilar.  For  this 
purpose  further  work  is  needed  to  devise  such  tests  as  are  re- 
lated to  the  types  of  experience  or  innate  development  as  are 
common  to  all  children  of  different  ages.  These  tests  must  also 
be  of  such  character  as  to  be  adapted  to  the  specific  opportunities 
for  specific  experiences  of  the  particular  atypical  child  to  be 
examined.  Careful  discrimination  on  the  part  of  the  examiner 
of  the  relation  of  the  tests  to  experience  will  prevent  such 
erroneous  conclusions  concerning  the  child's  ability  to  reason 
or  interpret  as  have  been  discussed  above  with  reference  to 
picture  tests,  definitions,  etc.  Tests  must  also  be  distinguished 
with  reference  to  the  two  phases  of  mental  activity  which  are 
concerned  in  a  judgment  of  general  intelligence.  These  are, 
as  has  already  been  intimated,  the  individual's  rate  of  acquisi- 
tion of  mechanical  learning  and  his  ability  to  apply  it  upon 
demand  to  a  new  situation. 

The  most  productive  source  for  such  study  is  probably  that 
presented  by  the  public  school.  Here  all  children  are  subjected 
to  the  same  set  of  experiences;  they  are  instructed  in  certain 
types  of  mechanical  acquisitions  such  as  the  recognition  of  words 
and  arithmetical  processes  which  they  are  expected  to  use  in 
further  acquisitions  of  knowledge.  The  reactions  of  normal 
and  atypical  children  to  the  school  situation  may  be  profitably 
analyzed  for  the  establishment  of  standards  of  normal  reaction 


STANDARDIZATION  OF  TESTS  173 

to  this  almost  universal  set  of  stimuli,  and  the  discrimination 
and  types  of  defective  reaction.  Such  work  upon  defective 
children  as  that  erf  Chotzen  (26)  can  be  of  little  value  until  this 
has  been  done.  The  children  studied  by  Chotzen  through  the 
medium  of  the  Binet-Simon  tests  were  selected  out  of  the  school 
and  segregated  in  the  Hilfsschule  because  of  their  defective 
or  atypical  reaction  to  the  school  situation.  It  would  seem  that 
it  is  only  an  analysis  of  this  or  other  types  of  social  reaction 
in  connection  with  the  many  other  factors  of  temperament,  home 
or  other  social  environment,  physical  condition,  etc.,  which  may 
be  of  value  for  diagnosis  with  reference  to  educational  treatment 
or  social  or  institutional  classification.  These  other  factors  it 
is  not  now  our  province  to  discuss  further  than  to  quote  from 
an  article  by  Katzenellenbogen  (27)  who  enumerated  many  of 
them  in  connection  with  the  discussion  of  the  relation  of  epilepsy 
to  mental  tests. 

"The  French  psychologists,  Binet  and  Simon,  prepared  this 
test  primarily  for  the  use  of  normal  children,  in  order  to  gain 
a  more  exact  and  uniform  basis  for  placing  children  in  the 
corresponding  school  grades.  The  necessary  assumption  for  the 
arrangement  was  the  uniformity  of  conditions,  under  which  a 
child  of  a  given  age  was  supposed  to  be.  A  child  for  instance, 
of  the  age  of  nine,  should  have  according  to  the  arrangement 
of  French  schools  such  and  such  knowledge;  the  tests  were 
made  a  posteriori  with  a  selection  of  the  highest  percentaged 
questions,  answered  by  children  of  a  given  age.  Having  a 
practical  point  of  view  in  mind,  this  test  has  been  of  the  greatest 
value  in  France  and  could  be  easily  applied  as  a  routine  ex- 
amination of  children  in  any  country,  with  the  necessary 
modifications.  Although  native  ability  plays  an  important  role 
in  such  a  test,  the  training  is  an  essential  condition  of  the 
child's  success.  If  a  child  failed,  and  was  retarded  for  in- 
stance two  years  the  Binet  test  would  diagnose  the  case  as 
"retarded,"  without  giving  the  cause  for  retardation.  Such  a 
retardation  might  be  due  to  mental  dullness — an  inborn  con- 
dition— or  to  lack  of  previous  educational  experiences,  to 
sickness,  adenoids,  psychopathic  timidity  and  nervousness,  or 


I74  CLARA    SCHMITT 

other  accidental  causes.  It  is  even  possible  that  one  should 
attribute  to  a  child  which  is  only  one  year  behind  his  normal 
according  to  the  Binet-Simon  test  an  especially  good  native  men- 
tality, as  he  in  spite  of  some  cause  (epilepsy  'for  instance,  as  we 
shall  later  have  the  opportunity  to  see)  has  fallen  no  more  than  a 
year  behind. 

"We  thus  see,  that  difficulties  arise  even  in  dealing  with  normal 
children  and  that  even  in  public  schools,  this  test  would  cause 
teachers  who  lack  a  psychological  or  more  important  even,  a 
medical  knowledge,  to  commit  graver  mistakes  with  the  Binet- 
Simon  test,  than  they  otherwise  would.  I  can  however  say, 
that  every  teacher' — provided  he  is  of  the  same  sex  as  the 
child, — would  be  able,  when  properly  instructed,  to  apply  the 
test  under  one  condition,  that  the  answers  should  be  recorded 
verbatim,  scored  with  the  aid  of  a  stop  watch.  The  real  prob- 
lem, however  should  begin  at  this  point,  and  every  child  having 
the  record  of  failure,  that  is  every  child  having  the  same  op- 
portunity as  the  others,  which  would  fail,  should  be  turned 
over  to  a  psychopathologist  (with  medical  knowledge)  who 
would  have  the  task  to  investigate  the  cause.  Thus  applied, 
the  Binet-Simon  test  even  in  its  present  form  would  be  of  great 
value,  as  it  would  lead  to  the  individualisation  of  pedagogical 
attention  and  would  result  in  proper  segregation.  Such  a  pro- 
cedure however,  would  under  the  present  arrangement  of  the 
test,  although  pedagogically  interesting  and  valuable,  be  of  little 
use  to  the  scientific  phase  of  the  problem. 

"The  apparent  success  of  this  test  with  normal  children  led 
the  French  psychologists  to  apply  it  as  primarily  devised  for 
pedagogical  purposes  and  for  the  use  of  normal  children,  to 
abnormal  ones.  They  did  not  even  stop  at  the  using  of  this 
test  upon  children,  but  thought  it  possible  to  apply  it  to  adults 
as  well.  The  necessary  hypothesis  which  led  them  to  such  an 
application  is  a  psychogenetic  point  of  view,  that  imbeciles 
reach  a  certain  maximum  of  development  and  then  stop  for 
ever  and  that  theerfore,  an  imbecile  of  twenty-five  years  may 
be  compared  with  a  child  of  five,  if  such  an  individual  fails 
in  the  test  devised  for  a  child  of  six.  Kuelpe,  justly  questions 


STANDARDIZATION  OF  TESTS  175 

this  point  of  view,  claiming  that  one  has  no  more  right  to 
compare  imbeciles  with  normal  children  of  a  certain  age,  than 
to  claim  that  dwarfs  are  physically  children  who  did  not  develop 
above  a  certain  age.  This  point  of  view  is  indeed  not  new. 
Wildermuth  tried  to  adapt  this  point  of  view  to  idiots,  but  failed 
to  convert  others  to  his  way  of  thinking.  Let  us  consider  the 
case  of  an  imbecile  of  eighteen,  whose  mental  age  according 
to  the  Binet-Simon  scale  is  six  years.  As  a  rule  such  a  low  grade 
imbecile  is  far  below  a  normal  child  of  that  age  regarding  adap- 
tability to  new  surroundings,  or  ability  for  learning  or  being 
trained.  He  may  exceed  on  the  other  hand  a  normal  child  in  the 
knowledge  of  money,  counting,  etc.,  which  knowledge  he  has 
acquired  during  the  twelve  years  of  additional  life.  Finally, 
the  sexual  maturity  will  disclose  a  new  life  of  inner  psychic 
experiences  unknown  to  the  child.  The  same  dissimilarity  exists 
between  a  child  and  a  dwarf,  who  is  rather  a  caricature  of  an 
adult's  body;  in  like  manner  the  imbecile's  mind  is  a  caricature 
of  a  normal  adult's  mind. 

"This  objection  to  a  grading  like  the  Binet-Simon  could  be 
overcome,  if  instead  of  years,  a  system  of  grading  independent 
of  age  would  be  substituted.  For  instance  a  given  complex  of 
tests  would  indicate  a  certain  grade  of  development.  The  same 
complex  with  graded  difficulty  would  indicate  a  higher  degree 
of  mentality.  Such  a  grading  would  necessarily  be  a  quantitative 
one  in  most  of  the  tests.  The  Binet  test  has  some  of  its  tests 
arranged  in  this  way,  such  as  for  instance  the  impressibility 
test  for  words  and  digits,  unfortunately  without  a  systematic 
arrangement.  In  such  an  arrangement  the  grading  would  also 
be  artificial,  but  it  would  do  away  with  the  confusion,  especially 
among  laymen,  that  imbeciles  are  children  with  a  stunted 
development. 

"Imbecility  is  a  collective  diagnosis  of  many  conditions  not 
only  dissimilar  regarding  etiology  but  also  in  its  manifestations. 
It  is  often  difficult  to  differentiate,  where  the  imbecility  ends 
and  the  normal  dulness  begins.  Psychiatry  calls  imbecility  an 
abnormal  state  of  mind  with  a  manifestation  of  inferior  in- 
tellect, a  state  which  is  either  of  congenital  origin  or  which 


i76  CLARA    SCHMITT 

had   its   origin   in   some   pathological   conditions   occurring   in 
earliest  childhood. 

"The  one  diagnostically  important  point  in  imbecility  is  the 
impossibility  of  any  marked  improvement  and  the  lack  of  the 
capacity  for  gaining  knowledge  by  experience.     The  memory 
and    ability    for  gaining    even    an    extraordinary    mechanical 
knowledge  of  facts  can  however  be  intact.     I  remember  seeing 
in  Rome,  N.  Y.,  at  the  Custodial  Institution,  an  imbecile  of  a 
very  low  grade,  who  was  able  to  recite  the  capitals  of  all  the 
States  and  knew  a  great  many  historical  data.     These  facts  he 
repeated  however  in  a  parrot-like  manner  without  inner  under- 
standing.    To  make  a  diagnostical  point  of  calling  imbecile  all 
those  who  are  three  years   retarded  in  the  Binet-Simon   test, 
means  to  create  a  new  diagnosis,  only  valuable  in  its  application 
to  the  Binet-Simon  test.     There  is  no  doubt,  that  there  is  a 
possibility  of  training  an  imbecile  along  some  lines,  where  me- 
chanical knowledge  or  memory  is  concerned,  so  that  an  individual 
whose   grading   was    dependent    on   mechanical    appliance    and 
diagnostication,  after  a  certain  training  would  test  perhaps  no 
more  than  a  year  or  two  behind  his  age  after  a  week's  training 
thus  reaching  the  degree  of  backwardness  or  dulness.     As  on 
the  other  hand,  a  dementing  process  may  set  in  slowly  in  a  child 
(juvenile  paralysis,  dementia  praecox,  impossible  to  perceive  in 
their  initial  stages,  without  mentioning  epilepsy)   a  previously 
normal  child  might  be  classified  as  an  imbecile.    As  we  previously 
mentioned,  imbecility  is  a  well  defined  although  not  always  an 
easy  diagnosis  and  the  Binet  test  is  not  adaptable  to  the  making 
of   a   diagnosis   of   imbecility.      Pedagogical  psychologists   too 
easily  lose  track  of  the  fact,  that  imbecility  is  a  term  of  path- 
ology and  not  simply  a  gradation  of  mental  ability.     Such  a 
diagnosis  should  be  left  entirely  to  a  medical  psychopathologist. 
We  do  not  make  a  diagnosis  based  on  the  tests  alone  but  on  the 
whole  clinical  picture  which  must  correspond  to   the   findings 
of  the  test." 


XII 
QUALITATIVE  CLASSIFICATION  OF  TESTS 

In  conformity  with  the  principle  of  the  qualitative  evaluation 
of  reaction  to  tests  of  mental  ability  the  Binet-Simon,  the  Healy- 
Fernald  and  tests  of  reading  and  arithmetic  are  classified  below 
to  indicate  the  mental  process  which  is  concerned  in  the  per- 
formance of  the  individual  tests.  This  classification  indicates 
levels  of  intellectual  ability  from  the  highest  to  the  lowest  levels, 
where  mental  defectiveness  belongs.  The  reasons  which  underlie 
this  classification  of  tests  appear  in  the  earlier  discussion  of 
this  paper. 

Level  I.    (Normal  Level) 

Process:  reasoning  or  adaptation  of  a  learned  content  to  suit 
a  changed  situation. 
Tests:  Counting  backward. 

Criticizes  absurd  phrases. 

Uses  three  given  words  in  a  sentence. 

Interprets  picture. 

Defines  abstract  terms. 

Derives  the  sense  of  a  mixed  sentence. 

Solves  paper  cutting  test. 

Reconstructs  a  triangle. 

Test  III,  construction  Puzzle  "A." 

Test  IV,  construction  Puzzle  "B." 

Test  V,     Puzzle  Box. 

Test  IX,  X,  and  XI,  Code  test  group. 

Distinguishes  between  morning  and  afternoon,  months 
of  year,  and  date  (when  conception  of  time  relationship 
is  involved). 

Interprets  figurative  literary  material. 

The  use  of  arithmetical  processes  for  the  solution  of  new 
problems. 


I78  CLARA    SCHM1TT 

Level  II 

Process;   Conscious   choice  of   several   possible   reactions   to 
correspond  to  certain  already  learned  classifications. 
Tests:  Compare  weights.    (Is  not  a  test  of  sensory  discrimina- 
tion, since  it  is  the  classification  heavy  and  light  which  is 

determined.) 

Distinguishes  between  morning  and  afternoon. 

Defines  concrete  nouns. 

Defines  abstract  terms. 

Compares  faces  from  aesthetic  standpoint. 

Describes  picture. 

Compares  two  remembered  objects. 

Indicates  omissions  in  pictures. 

Problem  questions. 

Gives  rhymes. 

Solves  problems  from  several  given  facts. 

Solves  question  concerning  President  and  King. 

Summarizes  observations  made  by  Hervieu. 

Tells  a  story  after  reading. 

Use  of  arithmetical  formulae  for  the  solution  of  type 
problems. 

Level  III.     (High  Grade  Defective) 

Process;    conscious   choice    or    arrangement    of    material    to 
correspond  to  a  model  concretely  presented. 
Tests:  Copies  square. 
Copies  lozenge. 
Game  of  patience. 
Indicates  omission  in  pictures. 
Test  I,     Introduction  Puzzle. 
Test  II,  Special  Picture  Puzzle. 

Level  IV. 

Process;  mechanical  or  rote  learning. 
Tests:  Tests  of  immediate  memory. 

Test  VIII,  learning  of  arbitrary  associations. 
Executes  three  commissions. 


STANDARDIZATION  OF  TESTS  179 

Naming  colors. 

Counting. 

Making  change. 

Recognition  of  money. 

Copies  drawings  from  memory. 

Months  of  year. 

Date. 

Recognition  of  printed  words. 

Arithmetical  processes. 

Levels  I  and  II  belongs  to  the  normal  grade  of  mental  ability. 
That  child  is  normal  or  socially  satisfactory  who  can  think  for 
himsel'f,  as  indicated  by  the  tests  of  Level  I,  or  who  learns 
with  facility  in  such  a  way  as  may  be  indicated  by  the  tests  of 
Level  II.  The  mental  processes  of  the  upper  grades  of  the 
defective  classes  are  made  upon  Levels  III  and  IV.  Though 
all  normal  individuals  in  every  type  of  mental  activity  must  at 
some  time  belong  to  Levels  III  and  IV,  it  is  characteristic  of 
the  normal  type  that  it  advances  beyond  this.  It  may  be  es- 
tablished as  the  characteristic  of  the  defective  type  that  he  does 
not  advance  in  a  general  way  above  these  levels.  Defective  in- 
dividuals may  vary  in  their  ability  to  pass  over  from  Level  III 
into  Level  II  in  certain  specific  ways.  The  defective  individual's 
activities  in  Level  II  take  place  only  after  such  prolonged  ex- 
perience of  the  specific  types  underlying  the  mental  processes 
involved  as  to  make  the  result  finally  approach  that  belonging 
to  Level  IV,  mechanical  or  rote  learning.  It  is  in  this  sense 
that  tests  should  be  evaluated  with  reference  to  age  and,  that, 
only  as  age  may  be  related  to  certain  types  of  experience. 

The  classification  may  be  continued  downward  to  include  more 
fundamental  processes,  such  as  sensory  discrimination,  divorced 
largely  from  such  constructive  activities  as  are  indicated  in 
Level  IV. 


XIII 

BIBLIOGRAPHY 

(1)  Report   of    Committee   on   Physical   and   Mental   Tests, 
Psych.  Rev.  II.  132. 

(2)  GILBERT,   J.   ALLEN.     Research  upon  the   Mental   and 
Physical  Development  of  School  Children.     Studies  from  Yale 
Psychological  Laboratory,  Vol.  II,  40. 

(3)  TOWNSEND,  PORTER  W.    The  Physical  Basis  of  Precocity 
and  Dullness.     Transactions  of  the  Academy  of  Science  of  St. 
Louis,  1893. 

(4)  S MEDLEY,  FRED  W.     Physical  Development  and  School 
Standing.    Report  of  Department  of  Child  Study  and  Pedagogic 
Research,  Chicago,  1900. 

(5)  WEST,  G.  M.     Observations  on  the  Relation  of  Physical 
Development  and  School  Standing.    Science,  n.  s.  4,  156. 

(6)  WHIPPLE,  GUY  M.    Reaction  Time  as  a  Test  of  Mental 
Ability.    AJ.P.  XV,  489. 

(7)  WISSLER,  CLARK.    Psych.  Rev.,  Ill,  Mon.  Sup.  6. 

(8)  GRIPPING,  HAROLD.    On  the  Development  of  Visual  Per- 
ception and  Attention,  A.J.P.,  VII,  227. 

(9)  SPEARMAN,  C.     General  Intelligence  Objectively  Deter- 
mined and  Measured.    A.J.P.,  15,  201. 

(10)  BURT,  CYRIL.     Experimental  Tests  of  General  Intelli- 
gence.   British  J.P.,  3,  94. 

(n)  BINET  AND  SIMON.  Le  developpement  de  1'intelligence 
chez  les  enfants,  L'Annee  Psychologique,  14,  i. 

Nouvelles  recherches  sur  la  mesure  du  niveau  intellectuel  chez 
les  enfants  d'ecole,  ditto,  17,  145. 

L'intelligence  of  imbeciles,  ditto,  15,  i. 

A  Method  of  Measuring  the  Development  of  the  Intelligence 
of  Young  Children,  Authorized  translation  by  Clara  Harrison 
Town. 

(12)  BOBERTAG,  OTTO.     Uber  Intelligenz  Priifungen.     Zeit. 
fur  Angew.    Psych.,  2:  105.    Uber  Intelligenz  Priifungen.  ditto, 
5 : 105,  and  6,  495. 

(13)  GODDARD,  H.  H.     Four  Hundred  Feeble  Minded  Chil- 
dren Classified  by  the  Binet  Method.     Ped.  Sem.  17:  387.    Two 
Thousand  Normal  Children  Measured  by  the  Binet  Measuring 
Scale  of  Intelligence,  Ped.  Sem.  18:232. 


STANDARDIZATION  OF  TESTS  181 

(14)  KUHLMAN,  H.     Results  oif  Grading  Thirteen  Hundred 
Feeble-minded  Children  with  the  Binet-Simon  Tests.     J.   Ed. 
Psych.,  4:261.     J.  Psych.  Aesth.,   16:173. 

(15)  TERMAN  AND  CHILDS.     A  Tentative  Revision  and  Ex- 
tension of  the  Binet-Simon   Measuring   Scale   of   Intelligence. 
J.  Ed.  Psych.  3:61,  133,  198,  277. 

(16)  SQUIRE,  CARRIE  RANSOM.    Graded  Mental  Tests.  J.  Ed. 
Psych. 

(17)  WALLIN,  J.  E.  W.     A  Practical  Guide  for  the  Admin- 
istration of  the  Binet-Simon  Scale  for  Measuring  Intelligence. 
Psych.  Cl.  5:217. 

(18)  SCHMITT,  CLARA.     The  Binet-Simon  Tests  of  Mental 
Ability.     Discussion  and  Criticism.     Ped.   Sem.   19:186. 

(19)  DOUGHERTY,  MARY  L.    Report  of  the  Binet-Simon  Tests 
Given  to  Four  Hundred  Eighty-Three  Children  in  the  Public 
Schools  of  Kansas  City,  Kansas.    J.  Ed.  Psych.,  14:338. 

(20)  HUEY,  EDMUND  BURK.     Backward  and  Feeble-minded 
Children. 

(21)  HOLMES,  ARTHUR.     The  Conservation  of  the  Child. 

(22)  BARR,  MARTIN  W.     Mental  Defectives,  Their  History 
and  Treatment. 

(23)  BINET  AND  SIMON.     Enfants  Anormaux. 

(24)  TREDGOLD,  A.  F.     Mental  Deficiency. 

(25)  STERN,  WM.     The  Psychological  Methods  of  Testing 
Intelligence. 

(26)  CHOTZEN,  F.     Die  Bedeutung  der  Intelligenz  Priifung- 
methode  von  Binet-Simon  bei  Schwachsinnigen  Kinder.     Zeit. 
Angew.   Psych.,  6:  411. 

(27)  KATZENELLENBOGEN,    EDWIN.      A    Critical    Essay    on 
Mental  Tests  in  Their  Relation  to  Epilepsy.     Epilepsia,  No.  2, 


PSYCHOLOGICAL  REVIEW  PUBLICATIONS  .      '     _. 

No.  4  Whole  No.  84 


THE 

Psychological  Monographs 

EDITED  BY 

JAMES  ROWLAND  ANGELL,  UNIVERSITY  OF  CHICAGO 
HOWARD  C.  WARREN,  PRINCETON  UNIVERSITY   (Index) 

JOHN  B.  WATSON,  JOHNS  HOPKINS  UNIVERSITY  (Review)  and 
SHEPHERD  I.  FRANZ,  Govr.  HOSP.  FOR  INSANE  (Bulletin) 


A  STUDY  OF 
RETROACTIVE  INHIBITION 

By 
J.  EDGAR  DECAMP,  Ph.  D. 


PSYCHOLOGICAL   REVIEW   COMPANY 

PRINCETON,  N.  J. 
AND   LANCASTER,   PA. 

AGENTS  :    G.  E.  STECHERT  &  CO.,  LONDON  (2  Star  Yard,  Carey  St.,  W.  C.) : 
LEIPZIG  (Koenig  Sir.,  37) ;    PARIS  (16,  Rue  de  Cond6) 


CONTENTS 

PAGE 

A.  Origin  of  the  Problem i 

B.  Work  of  others — Literature 2 

C.  Apparatus     8 

D.  Nonsense   Syllables — Their   Preparation n 

E.  General   Method    13 

F.  Experimental  Section   15 

Experiment  I    15 

Experiment  II   22 

Experiment  III    28 

Experiment  IV    30 

Experiment  V   31 

Experiment  VI    32 

Experiment  VII    33 

Experiment  VIII    35 

Experiment  IX(a)    36 

Experiment  IX (b)    50 

Experiment  X   52 

Experiment  XI    53 

Experiment  XII    54 

Experiment  XIII    57 

G.  General  Discussion    60 

H.     Summary  and  Conclusions 66 


A.     ORIGIN  OF  THE  PROBLEM 

Since  the  rather  exhaustive  work  of  Miiller  and  Pilzecker, 
investigating  various  aspects  of  memory,  Retroactive  Inhibition1 
has  held  an  unquestioned  place  among  the  principles  of  Psy- 
chology. It  follows  naturally  that  it  may  play  an  important  part 
in  our  mental  activities.  Undoubtedly,  it  is  to  be  considered 
in  the  construction  of  any  system  for  the  attainment  of  the 
highest  efficiency  in  learning  for  recall,  with  the  least  expenditure 
of  mental  energy.  Furthermore,  it  seems  in  accordance  with 
popular  science,  and  with  a  certain  scientific  tendency  towards 
the  assertion  of  the  unification  of  both  mental  and  physical 
energy,  that  if,  after  studying  one  task,  the  attention  is  turned 
to  a  second  task  immediately  upon  the  completion  of  the  first, 
the  first  task  will  not  have  the  same  recall  value  as  if  the  second 
task  had  not  succeeded  the  first. 

At  the  beginning  of  this  work  the  author  took  these  conclusions 
as  well  grounded  and,  sharing  the  consensus  of  opinion  among 
psychologists  that  the  effect  of  retroactive  inhibition  is  quite 
marked  under  certain  conditions,  proceeded  upon  this  basis.  In 
the  work  of  Miiller  and  Pilzecker  the  existence  of  retroactive 
inhibition  was  seemingly  definitely  established.  However,  into 
a  consideration  of  its  various  aspects  they  did  not  enter.  It 
seemed,  then,  of  scientific  value  to  know  the  more  minute  de- 
tails of  the  workings  of  this  process.  In  carrying  out  this 
work,  it  was  the  author's  purpose  to  investigate  the  nature  of 
the  working  of  retroactive  inhibition,  to  produce  curves  evidenc- 
ing its  relative  influence  with  the  lapse  of  time  subsequent  upon 
learning,  to  consider  the  nature  of  these  curves,  and  to  note 
important  subsidiary  phenomena  as  they  might  appear  in  the 
course  of  the  experiment. 

lFor  convenience  of  expression,  the  writer  deals  with  retroactive  inhibi- 
tion, throughout  this  paper,  as  if  it  were  really  denned  as  an  active  mental 
process,  rather  than  as  an  effect  of  some  activity  upon  the  permanence  of  a 
closely  preceding  activity. 


2  J.  EDGAR  DECAMP 

The  experimental  work  upon  this  problem  was  begun  in  the 
summer  of  1912.  The  work  done  at  that  time,  though  bearing 
out  the  general  conclusions  of  this  paper,  is  not  considered  suffi- 
ciently accurate  and  exhaustive  to  serve  as  a  base  for  any  definite 
conclusions;  the  development  of  a  more  accurate  method  being 
its  chief  service.  The  work  upon  which  our  conclusions  are 
based  was  carried  out  in  the  school  years  191 2-' 13,  191 3-' 14, 
and  the  summer  of  1913.  In  Experiments  I  to  XII  seven  sub- 
jects— six  men  and  one  woman — were  used.  In  Experiments 
XIII  34  subjects — eleven  women  and  twenty-three  men — were 
employed. 

B.  WORK  OF  OTHERS— LITERATURE 

From  1892  to  1900  Muller  and  Pilzecker,  two  German  inves- 
tigators, continued  the  work  on  memory,  begun  some  years  pre- 
viously by  Muller  and  Schumann.  They  conducted  a  series  of 
forty  experiments,  endeavoring  to  bring  out  the  main  laws  of 
association  and  inhibition  as  they  appear  in  connection  with  the 
learning  of  nonsense  material.  Their  report  of  these  experiments 
appeared  in  the  Zeitschrift  fur  Psychologie,  1900,  Erganzungs- 
band  I.  Their  work  is  an  example  of  untiring  effort  in  the 
quest  for  knowledge.  We  are  indebted  mostly  to  them  for  our 
present  conception  of  the  Perseveration  Tendency,  Generative, 
Effectual,  and  Retroactive  Inhibition.  It  is  the  last,  Retroactive 
Inhibition,  which  forms  the  main  subject  of  our  discussion  in 
this  paper.  Closely  related  to  our  phase  of  the  memory  problem 
they  conducted  nine  experiments,  (Versuchsreihen  29  to  37). 
The  general  tenor  of  their  method  in  these  experiments  is  as 
follows : 

The  subject  is  shown  a  series  of  twelve  nonsense  syllables,  writ- 
ten on  a  strip  of  paper,  and  placed  around  a  twelve-sided  prism 
in  such  a  way  that  a  syllable  corresponds  to  each  of  the  twelve 
sides  of  the  prism.  This  prism  is  revolved  behind  a  metal  screen, 
containing  an  adjustable  slit  through  which  the  subject  may 
view  the  syllables  as  they  pass  on  the  revolving  cylinder.  The 
syllables  are  repeated  a  certain  number  of  times  in  trochaic 
rhythm.  The  number  of  repetitions  for  any  experiment  is  kept 
constant.  The  variability  of  the  experiment  lies  in  the  arrange- 


A  STUDY  OF  RETROACTIVE  INHIBITION  3 

ment  of  the  mental  activity  of  the  subject  during  the  interval 
between  the  learning  and  the  test.  This  interval,  for  simplicity 
of  explanation,  may  be  divided  into  three  parts,  (i)  a  rest,  (2) 
mental  activity,  usually  consisting  of  an  after-series  (Nachreihe), 
i.e.  the  attention  is  turned  to  the  learning  of  a  new  series  of 
syllables,  and  (3)  a  second  rest  period.  Then  follows  the  test. 
For  testing  the  syllables,  the  apparatus  mentioned  above,  to- 
gether with  a  Hipp  chronoscope  and  lip-key,  or  sound-key 
(Schalltrichter),  are  introduced  into  an  electric  circuit.  The 
chronoscope  is  adjusted  for  running  with  a  broken  circuit.  A 
slide,  to  hide  the  syllable  from  the  subject's  view,  is  held  in 
front  of  the  slit  by  means  of  an  electro-magnet  just  above  it. 
The  operator  breaks  the  circuit,  allowing  the  slide  to  fall,  thus 
exposing  the  syllable  to  toe  tested.  With  the  fall  of  the  slide  the 
chronoscope  starts.  The  subject  reacts  with  what  he  thinks  to 
be  the  corresponding  unaccented  syllable.  If  he  does  not  know, 
he  replies  "Nichts."  A  brief  outline  of  eight  of  the  nine  ex- 
periments (Versuchsreihen  29  to  37)  follows.  The  other, 
because  of  its  important  bearing  upon  the  problem  in  hand,  is 
given  in  toto. 

Experiments  29  and  30: 

The  purpose  of  these  two  experiments  was  a  comparison  of 
24-hour  and  n -minute  associations.  Little  more  was  accom- 
plished than  a  mere  notice  by  one  of  the  subjects  that  employment 
duringjrie  rest  interval  after  learning  a  set  of  syllables.  Jnjured 
the  associations  of  these  syllables.  The  numerical  results  showed 
nothing  of  particular  importance. 

Experiment  31 : 

In  this  experiment  a  comparison  was  made  between  series 
learned  with  no  mental  activity  closely  following,  and  series 
learned  with  the  learning  of  a  second  series  following  34.4  sec- 
onds after  the  completed  learning  of  the  first.  In  the  first 
case  the  average  success  was  48  per  cent,  with  the  average  time 
for  successful  scores  24800-.  The  corresponding  figures  for  the 
other  case  were  23  per  cent  and  35700-.  The  number  of  accented 
syllables  shown  in  the  test  was  144. 


4  /.   EDGAR  DECAMP 

(For  brevity  the  following  symbols  are  brought  over  from 
Miiller  and  Pilzecker's  work.  H(Hauptreihe),  a  series  learned 
for  test.  N(Nachreihe),  a  series  learned  soon  after  the  learn- 
ing of  an  H  series.  V(Vergleichsreihe),  a  series  with  no  N 
series  following  its  learning;  r,  the  per  centum  of  successes;  Tr, 
the  average  time  for  the  successes ;  n,  the  total  number  of  syllables 
tested  in  any  particular  experiment.) 

Experiment  32 : 

The  general  plan  was  the  same  as  that  of  Experiment  31. 
The  reading  of  the  N  series  was  begun  17.8  sees,  after  learning 
an  H  series.  The  results,  n  =  162,  were:  For  the  H  series; 
r  —  27  per  cent,  Tr  =  32300-;  For  the  V  series;  r  =  55  per  cent, 
Tr  =  30700-.  The  subject  indicated  that  he  would  rather  not 
look  at  a  newspaper,  for  it  would  make  him  forget  his  syllables. 
He  found  the  best  way  to  'employ  his  time  was  to  walk  up 
and  down  the  room,  and  give  himself  up  to  meditation. 

Experiment  33 : 

Plan  of  Experiment  32.  The  reading  of  the  N  series 
was  begun  27  sees,  after  the  reading  of  the  H  series.  The 
experiment  was  discontinued  after  eight  days,  "well  die  riick- 
wirkende  Hemmung  in  unbestreitbarer  Weise  hervorgetreten  war 
und  eine  weitere  Fortzetzung  der  Versuche  uberfliissig  erschien." 
For  the  H  series ;  r  =  43  per  cent,  Tr  —  2260  o- .  For  the  V  series ; 
r  —  72  per  cent,  Tr  —  20900-.  (n  =  72). 

Experiment  34 : 

This  experiment  was  designed  to  compare  two  kinds  of  series, 
and  Hj  series,  where  the  N  series  followed  after  17.2  sees.,  and 
H2  series,  where  the  N  series  followed  after  six  minutes. 
The  accented  syllables  of  the  Hx  and  the  H2  series  were  mixed 
and  tested  after  one  and  one-half  hours.  Results:  For  the  HI 
series;  r  =  28  per  cent,  Tr  =  27600-.  For  the  H2  series;  r  —  49 
per  cent,  Tr  =  30000-.  (n  =  144). 

The  subject  stated  that  it  was  better  not  to  busy  herself 
with  a  magazine  during  the  rest  period,  for  she  had  previously 
noticed  that  such  activity  was  injurious  to  the  recall  of  the 
syllables.  It  was  best  to  allow  the  thoughts  to  wander  where 
they  would. 


A  STUDY  OF  RETROACTIVE  INHIBITION  5 

Experiment  35  (in  toto.1) 

This  experiment,  in  which  Dr.  Behrens  acted  as  subject,  was 
conducted  in  the  same  way  as  Experiment  32,  in  which  Dr. 
Behrens  was  the  subject,  with  the  exception  of  the  unimportant 
circumstance,  that  the  main  series  (Hauptreihe)  and  the  com- 
parison series  (Vergleichsreihe)  were  not  read  twelve  times  but, 
on  account  of  the  acuteness  of  the  subject,  were  read  only  eight 
times;  and  with  the  exception  of  the  very  important  circum- 
stance that  the  mental  activity  of  the  subject,  following  the 
reading  of  the  main  series,  did  not  consist  in  the  reading  of  a 
second  series  of  syllables.  The  subject  had,  directly  after  the 
reading  of  each  main  series,  to  observe  attentively  three  different 
landscape  pictures  that  had  been  previously  prepared  for  the 
purpose,  and,  directly  after  the  observation  of  all  three  pictures, 
to  describe,  as  nearly  correctly  as  possible,  to  the  operator  what 
he  had  seen  in  each  picture.  The  time  of  observation  for  each 
picture  lasted  ten  seconds.  To  be  sure,  a  picture  once  used  was 
never  used  again  in  the  experiment.  In  order  that  the  considera- 
tion of,  and  the  meditation  on,  the  pictures  might  not  leave  behind 
any  fatigue  of  the  subject  for  the  test  of  the  just  previously  read 
syllables  of  the  main  series,  the  work  with  the  pictures  never 
extended  over  a  longer  interval  than  two  minutes,  so  that,  if 
the  subject  had  not  finished  the  picture  work  after  a  lapse  of 
two  minutes  after  completing  the  reading  of  the  main  series, 
he  was  stopped  after  this  period  without  further  ado.  Between 
the  picture  work  and  the  test  of  the  accented  syllables  of  the 
main  series  there  always  occurred  a  rest  period  of  at  least  six 
minutes.  The  experiment  was  discontinued  after  twelve  days 
for  the  expected  result  was  obtained  in  an  unquestioned  way. 
The  main  series,  whose  readings  the  picture  work  followed, 
gave  with  each  temporal  position  many  less  successes  and  fewer 
very  small  times  than  the  comparison  series  which  were  not 
followed  by  picture  work.  On  the  whole,  n  =  108,  there  resulted : 

For  the  H  series ;  r  =  24  per  cent,  Tr  =  29500- 
For  the  V  series ;  r  =  56  per  cent,  Tr  =  24900-. 

1  Free  translation. 


6  /.  EDGAR  DECAMP 

If  one  compares  these  results  with  those  received  in  Experi- 
ment 32,  it  appears  that  in  this  experiment  with  picture 
work,  at  least  as  strong  an  injurious  influence  upon  the  associa- 
tions was  at  work  as  that  which  existed  in  Experiment  32  through 
the  i2-time  reading  of  the  after  series.  We  hasten  to  remark 
that,  before  the  beginning  of  this  experiment,  we  knew,  through 
occasional  experiments,  Dr.  Behrens  might  possess  a  strong  in- 
terest and  concentrated  attention  with  visual  pictures.  It  does 
not  necessarily  follow,  that  other  subjects,  in  experiments  ar- 
ranged as  ours,  may  not  give  less  striking  results. 

Experiment  36: 

The  savings  method  was  used  with  the  learning  of  another 
set  of  syllables  for  the  mental  activity  following  the  learning 
of  the  main  series.  Results  in  two  cases  were  obtained,  (n=32)  : 

An  H  series,  requiring  an  average  of  13  repetitions. 

A  V  series,  requiring  an  average  of  11.25  repetitions. 

An  H  series,  requiring  an  average  of  7.8  repetitions. 

A  V  series,  requiring  an  average  of  6.6  repetitions. 

Experiment  37: 

The  savings  method  was  used.  As  in  Experiment  35  pic- 
tures were  used  for  influencing  the  fixity  of  the  associations 
of  the  previously  learned  series  of  syllables.  Results,  n  =  24 : 

An  H  series,  requiring  an  average  of  8.0  repetitions. 

A  V  series,  requiring  an  average  of  4.9  repetitions. 
Conclusions  of  Miiller  and  Pilzecker  (abridged)  : 
v     i.  The  introspections  of  the  subjects  indicate  retroactive  in- 
hibition. 

2.  The  numerical  results  of  Experiments  31  to  37  indicate 
retroactive  inhibition. 

^3.  Retroactive  inhibition  is  the  greater  the  more  attention  paid 
to  the  interpolated  work. 

\f  4.  Retroactive  inhibition  is  much  weaker  when  the  interpolated 
work  is  done  after  six  minutes  than  when  it  is  done  immediately 
after  learning. 

5.  That  the  work  precludes  thinking  of  the  syllables  does 
not  explain  retroactive  inhibition. 


A  STUDY  OF  RETROACTIVE  INHIBITION  7 

6.  The  results  obtained  are  not  due  to  the  similarity  of  the 
syllables  of  the  N  series  to  those  of  the  SELseriesr^AJ". 

7.  The  influence  of  fatigue,  following  the  work,  was  eliminated. 

8.  The  work  did  more  than  merely  to  weaken  the  condition 
of  preparedness. 

9.  Physiological   activity   continues   after   reading.      This   is 
weakened  by  any  other  activity. 

~~io.  The  processes  underlying  retroactive  inhibition   and  the 
perseveration  tendency  are  the  same. 

11.  The  objection  that,  because  of  the  perseveration  tendency, 
the  associations  should  increase,  is  not  valid. 

12.  The  condition  of  preparedness  between  the  first  and  the 
eighth  syllables   is  different   from   that  between  the   first   and 
second,  for  the  brain  excitations  are  different. 

13.  The  application  to  didactics  is  clear. 

At  the  Fifth  International  Congress  for  Experimental  Psy- 
chology,1 1912,  G.  E.  Miiller  made  a  brief  report  of  an  investi- 
gation which  was  then  being  carried  on  by  Frl.  Heine,  who 
was  working  under  the  direction  of  Professor  Miiller.  Her  re- 
sults with  nine  subjects  indicated  that  retroactive  inhibition  does 
not  apply  to  the  process  of  Recognition,  when  tested  with  nonsense 
syllables.  Four  of  the  subjects  were  tested  for  retroactive  in- 
hibition, according  to  the  method  used  by  Miiller  and  Pilzecker. 
The  average  percentages  of  the  four  tests  for  the  Hauptreihen 
and  the  Vergleichsreihen  were  22  per  cent  and  38  per  cent 
respectively. 

Muller  remarked  that,  from  the  results  obtained,  it  appeared 
retroactive  inhibition  manifested  itself  less  easily  as  more  repe- 
titions were  given  the  series  in  the  learning  process. 

Other  than  the  work  of  Muller  and  Pilzecker  and  that  of 
Frl.  Heine,  the  writer  is  unaware  of  any  important  work  done 
upon  the  subject  of  retroactive  inhibition.  As  noted  by  Muller 
and  Pilzecker  and  by  Muller,  a  few  investigators,  Bigham,  Von 
Kries,  E.  Meyer,  in  experiments  with  different  purposes  in  view, 
have  made  mention  of  results,  more  or  less  harmonizing  with 

1  Vide  Bcricht  iiber  den  V .  Kongress  f.  exper.  Psychologic,  1912,  S.  216  ff. 


8  /.   EDGAR   DECAMP 

the  idea  of  retroactive  inhibition.  Bigham,1  in  the  presentation 
of  numbers,  colors,  forms,  syllables,  etc.,  noticed  more  errors 
after  an  attention-engaged  interval,  and  remarked,  "The  filling 
of  the  intervals  hinders  the  memory."  Von  Kries2  remarked, 
that  the  memory  of  a  seen  extent  fades  very  quickly,  if  the  time 
following  the  perception  of  the  extent  is  filled  with  some  other 
activity. 

Some  of  the  recent  texts  on  Psychology  call  attention  to  retro- 
active inhibition.  Pillsbury,3  in  his  Essentials;  Meumann,4  in  his 
book  on  memory ;  and  Myers,5  in  his  brief  summary  of  the  work 
done  on  memory,  allude  to  its  existence.  Ebbinghaus6  refers  to 
it  as  a  possible  explanation  for  the  disproportionate  increase  in 
the  number  of  repetitions  required  as  the  "memory  span"  is 
exceeded. 

C.     APPARATUS 

The  apparatus  used  in  the  majority  of  these  experiments  may 
conveniently  be  divided  into  three  parts. 

i.  A  modified  form  of  the  Wirth  card-exposure  apparatus. 
This  apparatus  was  modified  and  used  by  Dr.  J.  F.  Shepard  and 
H.  M.  Fogelsonger  in  their  work  on  Inhibition.  In  their  article 
reference  is  made  to  the  main  modification.7  For  clearness  the 
modifications  there  introduced  may  be  briefly  described.  In 
place  of  the  cylinder  with  stationary  electrical  contacts,  an  all- 
metal  cylinder,  with  adjustable  contacts,  was  substituted.  Around 
the  cylinder,  and  beneath  a  surface  shell,  were  placed  fourteen 
sliding  strips,  each  strip  bearing  a  metal  contact  point  projecting 
through  a  narrow  groove  in  the  surface  shell.  By  adjusting  these 
strips,  any  number  of  the  fourteen  points  can  be  arranged  so  that 
contact  with  the  insulated  projecting  brush  will  be  made  as  the 
cylinder  revolves.  Midway  between  two  strips  was  soldered, 

1  Cited  from  Miiller  and  Pilzecker's  articles,  S.  194. 

2  Cited  from  Miiller  and  Pilzecker's  article,  S.   194. 

3  Essentials  of  Psychology,  1913,  p.  196. 

4  The  Psychology  of  Learning,  English  translation,  1912,  p.  147. 
"Text-book  of  Experimental  Psychology,  1911,  Pt.  I,  p.  153  f. 

8  Grundzuge  der  Psychologic,  1902,  S.  652. 

7  Psychological  Review,  Vol.  XX,  'No.  4,  July,  1913,  p.  292. 


A  STUDY  OF  RETROACTIVE  INHIBITION  9 

along  its  middle  line,  leaving  its  edges  free,  a  thin  and  narrow 
strips  This  resulted  in  fourteen  grooves,  directly  over  the  four- 
teen sliding  strips,  which  serve  to  hold  strips  of  paper  bearing 
syllables  for  the  exposure.  This  modification  greatly  facilitated 
the  manipulation  of  the  syllables. 

A  second  modification  consisted  in  the  addition  of  a  clutch 
to  take  the  place  of  the  cumbersome  link-chain  part.  In  using 
the  link-chain,  when  more  than  a  certain  number  of  revolutions 
of  the  cylinder  were  required,  the  operator  had  to  stop  in  the 
midst  of  the  presentation  process  until  the  chain  could  be  re- 
adjusted. With  the  clutch,  all  that  is  necessary,  when  the  weight 
runs  down,  is  to  gently  pull  the  end  of  the  cord  bearing  the 
counterweight.  The  regularity  of  the  revolution  of  the  cylinder 
is  not  altered,  and  the  distraction  of  the  subject,  due  to  the 
change,  is  negligible. 

A  further  and  minor  modification  was  the  addition  of  a  metal 
projection  to  each  of  the  two  ratchets,  thereby  enabling  the 
cylinder  to  be  easily  tripped  with  the  fingers,  when  it  is  not 
desired  to  use  the  electric  circuit  and  metronome  for  tripping. 

2.  A  Hipp  chronoscope  for  registering  time. 

3.  A  mouth-key.     The  mouth-key  served  as  a  reaction  part 
for  breaking  the  electric  circuit  upon  the  subject's  receiving  the 
stimulus  word.     This  apparatus  consists  of  a  circular  metal  box, 
somewhat  similar  to  a  frustum  of  a  cone,  the  small  end  of  which 
contains  an  opening.     The  sides  of  this  opening  are  shaped  to 
fit  snugly  about  the  nose  and  mouth.     Absorbent  cotton  was 
used  about  the  edges  to   render  the  box  air-tight  when  fitted 
against  the  face.    The  opposite  end  of  the  box  contains  a  single 
circular  opening  with  a  projecting  band.      Over  this  opening 
is  placed  a  rubber  tambour.     To  the  tambour  is  waxed  a  disk 
to  which  is  pivoted  an  aluminum  pointer,  one  end  of  which  is 
pivoted  to  a  post  on  the  side  of  the  box.     For  the  other  end 
of  the  pointer  there  is  an  insulated  post  upon  which  the  free 
end  of  the  pointer  may  rest.    Adjacent  to  the  insulated  post  is  a 
second  post  in  connection  with  the  box  itself.     These  posts  are 
fitted  for  wire  connection.     The  working  of  the  instrument  is 


io  /.  EDGAR  DECAMP 

as  follows :  When  the  mouth  and  nose  are  placed  in  the  opening, 
the  face  pressed  closely  against  the  edges  of  the  box,  and  a 
word  spoken,  the  air  pressure  forces  the  tambour  outward,  con- 
sequently pushing  the  pointer  off  its  post,  thereby  breaking  the 
circuit.1 

A  simple  metronome  for  regulating  the  rate  of  exposure  of 
the  syllables  and  a  commutator  complete  the  list  of  apparatus. 

The  complete  set-up  may  be  diagrammatically  represented  thus, 
Fig.  i. 

M.  K.,  mouth-key;  C,  commutator;  W.  C.  A.,  Wirth  card- 
exposure  apparatus  (modified)  ;  H.  C.,  Hipp  chronoscope. 

The  working  of  the  combined  apparatus,  in  a  test,  may  be 
briefly  mentioned.  The  syllables  to  be  tested  are  placed  on  the 
cylinder  of  W.  C.  A.  in  alternate  grooves.  One  of  the  two 
slides  closing  the  exposure  slit  should  be  pulled  up,  allowing 
the  subject  to  see  the  first  syllable  of  each  pair  of  syllables  as 
the  cylinder  revolves.  With  the  exposure  of  each  test  syllable 
contact  is  made  at  W.  C.  A.  Upon  thinking  of  a  suitable  re- 
sponse, the  subject  speaks  into  M.  K.  We  then  have  the  time 
elapsing  between  the  exposure  of  the  test  syllable  and  the  subject's 
reaction  thereto. 

1  The  mouth-key  was  designed  by  Dr.  Shepard  for  reducing,  as  far  as 
possible,  the  time  elapsing  .between  the  subject's  thought  of  a  response  and  his 
actual  response  as  indicated  by  the  apparatus.  The  lip-key  is  open  to  the 
objection  that  the  quickness  of  its  release  may  vary  with  the  response-word. 
In  fact,  a  clear  enunciation  of  some  words  does  not  necessarily  require  its 
release. 

The  sound  apparatus  has  the  objection  that  for  its  use  a  relay  must  be 
inserted.  This  latter  instrument  increases  the  inertia  to  be  overcome  and 
movement  to  be  made,  both  requiring  time.  The  mouth-key  is  scarcely  open 
to  either  of  these  objections.  When  the  face  is  held  tightly  against  the  open- 
ing the  initiation  of  any  sound,  even  a  whisper,  affects  the  tambour  through 
the  intervening  column  of  air.  However,  it  has  the  objection  that,  for  fairly 
long  times,  the  subject  can  not  maintain  his  position,  since  breathing  is 
necessarily  stopped.  Consequently,  in  long  times,  the  subject  must  allow  space 
between  the  face  and  the  instrument  for  breathing.  When  the  response  is 
thought  of,  movement  towards  the  instrument  must  be  made.  It  is  question- 
able, then,  whether  the  difficulty  has  'been  wholly  obviated. 


A  STUDY  OF  RETROACTIVE  INHIBITION 


ii 


FIG.  i 

D.     NONSENSE  SYLLABLES— THEIR  PREPARATION 

In  the  preparation  of  the  nonsense  syllables,  the  five  vowels, 
a,  e,  i,  o,  and  u,  and  all  the  consonants,  were  used.  A  nonsense 
syllable  was  composed  of  three  letters,  a  vowel  between  two 
consonants.  All  syllables  possible  with  the  various  combinations 
of  vowels  and  consonants  were  made.  Any  syllable  making 
a  sense  word  was  at  once  rejected.  Syllables  with  the  initial 
and  final  consonant  the  same  were  rejected.  The  remaining 
syllables — about  1400  in  number — were  written  on  slips  of  paper, 
placed  in  a  bag,  and  thoroughly  shuffled.  They  were  then  drawn 
by  lot.  Fourteen  syllables  were  arranged  in  pairs,  composing 
seven  measures  of  two  syllables  each,  with  the  following  restric- 
tions in  mind : 

1.  The  initial  consonant,  the  vowel,  and  the  final  consonant 
of  the  first  syllable  should  be  different  from  the  corresponding 
letters  of  the  second  syllable  of  the  same  measure. 

2.  The  final  consonant  of  the  first  syllable  of  a  measure  should 
be  different  from  the  initial  conspnant  of  the  second  syllable  of 
the  same  measure. 

3.  Two  syllables  having  an  apparent  sense  association  were 
not  used  to  form  a  measure. 


12  /.   EDGAR  DECAMP 

4.  In  the  seven  syllables,  occupying  the  first-syllable  position 
of  the  seven  measures,  all  the  vowels  were  represented,  and  two 
vowels  were  represented  twice.     The  same  applies  to  the  seven 
syllables  occupying  the  second-syllable  position.    The  two  vowels 
represented  twice  in  the   first  position  were  not  the  same  as 
those  represented  twice  in  the  second  position. 

5.  The   initial   letters   of   the   seven   syllables   occupying  the 
second-syllable  position  in  the  seven  measures  were  all  different. 
In  the  case  of  those  occupying  the  first  position,  not  more  than 
two  were  alike. 

6.  The  seven  measures  were  arranged  for  learning  so  that 
successive  vowels  of  the  different  syllables  were  not  the  same, 
and  initial  consonants  of  successive  syllables  were  different.    The 
end  consonant  of  one   syllable  was   different   from  the   initial 
consonant  of  the  succeeding  one. 

7.  In  a  set  of  fourteen  syllables,  two  syllables,  with  two  letters 
of  one  syllable  the  same  as  the  corresponding  letters  of  the  other, 
were  not  permitted. 

The  two  syllables  forming  a  measure  were  type-written  upon 
slips  of  paper  to  fit  the  grooves  of  the  exposure  apparatus.  The 
initial  letter  of  each  syllable  was  a  capital,  the  other  two  being 
small  pica  letters.  The  spacing  between  two  syllables  of  a 
measure  was  2  cm. 

In  any  experiment,  when  the  stock  of  syllables  was  exhausted, 
new  syllables  were  formed  by  dividing  the  total  number  of 
measures  into  three  parts,  the  measures  the  subject  first  learned 
being  used  as  the  first  part  for  forming  new  syllables.  These 
measures  were  mixed  thoroughly  and  drawn  by  lot,  and  new 
measures  of  syllables  formed  with  the  above  seven  restrictions 
in  mind,  and  with  the  further  restriction  that  two  syllables,  once 
forming  a  measure,  were  not  used  again  for  a  measure.  Usually 
the  syllable  occupying  the  first  (second)  half  of  a  measure  was 
used  for  the  second  (first)  half  of  a  new  measure. 

In  a  few  instances  errors  of  construction  crept  in  but  may 
be  considered  as  negligible.  It  may  be  remarked  that  absolute 
equality  of  different  series  of  syllables  appears  almost  impossible. 


A  STUDY  OF  RETROACTIVE  INHIBITION  13 

Considering  the  matter  of  associations  alone,  a  series  of  syllables, 
to  the  operator  apparently  free  from  associations,  often  occasions 
familiar  associations  to  the  subject.  These  can  scarcely  be 
avoided,  since  the  associations  of  two  individuals  are  often  quite 
different,  thus  making  it  impossible  for  one  to  arrange  wholly 
unassociated  material  for  the  other. 

E.     GENERAL  METHOD 

The  general  method  of  our  Experiments  I  to  XII  may  be 
briefly  outlined.  The  subject,  seated  in  front  of  the  apparatus, 
is  shown  a  series  of  fourteen  syllables,  seven  measures,  two 
syllables  to  the  measure.  The  cylinder  bearing  the  syllables  is 
revolved  by  the  operator  at  a  uniform  rate  throughout  the  expos- 
ing process.  The  cylinder  contains  fourteen  grooves  for  meas- 
ures. Only  alternate  grooves  are  filled  with  measures.  Each 
empty  groove  is  exposed  the  same  time  as  a  filled  one.  Thereby 
the  subject  sees  a  pair  of  syllables  for  a  certain  time  and,  follow- 
ing this  exposure,  there  is  a  rest  interval  of  the  same  length. 
The  subject  repeats  the  syllables  aloud  as  they  pass.  Equal  times 
of  exposure  are  obtained  by  the  use  of  the  metronome.  Following 
the  learning  is  an  interval  which  may  or  may  not,  according  to 
the  day's  position  in  the  experiment,  contain  a  sub-interval  of 
mental  activity.  When  this  sub-interval  was  employed  in  mental 
activity,  the  signal  used  for  the  subject  to  begin  was,  "Work," 
spoken  by  the  operator  at  the  desired  time.  When  the  operator 
desired  the  subject  to  cease  working,  this  was  indicated  by  simply 
saying,  "Rest."  How  much  mental  activity  had  to  be  engaged 
in,  following  the  learning,  was  kept  from  the  subject  as  nearly 
as  possible.  At  the  end  of  the  interval,  varying  in  length  accord- 
ing to  the  experiment,  came  the  test.  While  the  subject  was 
working  or  resting,  as  the  case  might  be,  the  operator  re-arranged 
the  measures  on  the  cylinder  in  an  order  different  from  that 
used  in  learning  them.  In  the  first  test  of  each  day  the  order 
of  test  of  the  measures,  as  compared  with  the  exposure  of  them, 
was  5,  7,  i,  3,  6,  2,  4.  In  the  second  test  of  the  day  the  order 
was  6,  2,  4,  7,  i,  3,  5.  Only  the  first  syllable  of  each  measure 


14  /.  EDGAR  DECAMP 

was  shown  for  test.  The  method  of  test  has  been  given  under 
Apparatus.  The  operator  notes  the  reply  of  the  subject  to  each 
syllable  shown,  records  the  time,  and  adjusts  the  apparatus. 
The  subject,  each  time  after  he  has  replied,  makes  the  contact 
at  the  mouth-key  for  the  next  test.  After  the  seven  syllables 
are  tested  there  follows  a  rest  period,  during  which  the  operator 
records  the  introspections  of  the  subject,  and  arranges  a  new 
set  of  syllables  on  the  cylinder  for  the  .second  learning,  which 
follows  after  a  rest  period  of  not  less  than  five  minutes.  Usually 
two  series  are  learned  and  tested  on  any  experiment  day.  The 
experiments  on  different  days  were  made  at  as  nearly  the  same 
hour  of  the  day  as  possible. 

In  recording  the  responses  of  the  subject  to  the  various  test 
syllables,  it  is  to  be  noted  that  the  first  response — the  one  giving 
the  time — is  always  used.  A  second  response,  though  correct, 
is  recorded  but  never  considered  as  a  correct  response  to  the 
test  syllable.  The  time  given  the  subject  to  think  of  a  suitable 
response  is  necessarily  restricted  to  the  limit  of  the  chronoscope, 
which  runs  apporoximately  one  minute.  It  may  be  noted  in  ad- 
vance that  such  long  times  are  exceedingly  rare. 

The  seven  subjects1  serving  in  the  major  portion  of  this 
work  were: 

Prof.  W.  B.  Pills'bury,  (P.) 

Dr.  H.  F.  Adams,  (A.) 

and  Graduate  Students, 

Miss  Z.  P.  Buck,  (Z.  B.) 

Mr.  W.   H.   Batson,  (B.) 

Mr.  F.  C.  Dockeray,  (D.) 

Mr.  H.  V.  Foulk,  '  (F.) 

Mr.  C.  P.  Wang,  (W.) 


1  To  some  of  these  the  writer  is  particularly  indebted  in  that  he  was  unable 
to  reciprocate  their  service. 


A  STUDY  OF  RETROACTIVE  INHIBITION  15 

F.     EXPERIMENTAL  SECTION 
EXPERIMENT  I 

This  experiment  extended  over  a  period  of  thirty-two  experi- 
ment days.  Successive  experiment  days  do  not  necessarily  coin- 
cide with  successive  calendar  days,  though  for  the  most  part  they 
do.  On  each  experiment  day  two  series  of  seven  measures 
each,  two  nonsense  syllables  to  the  measure,  were  shown  to 
the  subject.  Sixteen  repetitions  were  given  for  the  learning. 
The  rate  of  the  revolving  cylinder  was  kept  constant  by  the 
use  of  a  metronome.  The  metronome  was  set  at  seventy-two  beats 
a  minute.  On  each  second  beat  of  the  metronome  the  operator 
allowed  the  cylinder,  bearing  the  syllables,  to  move  arounld 
one  point.  Thus  a  measure  was  exposed  to  the  view  of  the 
subject  one  and  two-thirds  seconds,  then  a  blank  space  was  ex- 
posed for  one  and  two-thirds  seconds,  then  another  measure  for 
one  and  two-thirds  seconds,  and  so  on,  until  the  requisite  number 
of  repetitions  was  given.  This  process  is  designated  the  learning. 
Following  the  learning  there  elapsed  a  period  of  time  which  we 
shall  designate  throughout  as  the  variant  interval.  In  the  present 
experiment  this  interval  lasted  fifteen  minutes.  The  mental  ac- 
tivity of  the  subject  during  this  interval  is  the  important  variable 
in  our  experiment.  In  this  consists  our  variable  factor,  it  being 
our  purpose  to  keep,  from  day  to  day,  all  other  factors  as  nearly 
constant  as  possible.  The  plan  relative  to  the  disposition  of  this 
interval  for  the  different  days  may  be  given  as  follows : 

Series  A  Series  B 

Exp.  Day  Rest  Work  Rest  Work 

1  15  o  o  15 

2  14  I  I  14 

3  U  2  2  13 

4  12  3  3  12 


16  o  15  15  o 

17  o  15  15  o 

18  i  14  14  i 

19  2  13  13  2 


32  15  o  o  15 


16  J.  EDGAR  DECAMP 

The  caption  "Series  A"  is  used  to  designate  the  first  series 
learned  on  any  particular  day.  "Series  B"  refers  to  the  second 
series  of  the  same  day.  The  total,  as  given  above,  we  arbitrarily 
call  a  cycle  of  experiment  days.  Four  results  are  obtained  for 
any  definite  position  in  the  variant  interval,  e.g.,  take  rest  four 
and  work  twelve;  this  combination  occurs  four  times  in  the 
cycle.  It  will  be  noted  that  this  cyclic  arrangement  presents 
the  following  advantages: 

1.  The  effect  of   any  practice   is   fairly  equally   distributed. 
Thus  it  will  be  seen  that  the  number  of  days'  practice  preceding 
any  position  in  the  variant  interval  is  31.    E.g.,  suppose  we  take 
rest  12,  work  3.     The  days  preceding  the  first  result  are  three, 
those  preceding  the  last  result  are  twenty-eight. 

2.  The  effect  of  fatigue  is  balanced.     Any  criticism  that  the 
B  series  had  the  disadvantage,  because  of  the  learning  and  test 
of  the  preceding  A  series,  is  evaded,  at  least  as  far  as  the  com- 
parative value  of  the  results  is  concerned.     In  a  complete  cycle 
any  position  in  the  variant  interval  has  occurred  twice  in  the 
A  and  twice  in  the  B  series. 

During  that  part  of  the  variant  interval  not  taken  up  with 
work,  the  subject  sat  quietly,  having  been  instructed  not  to  engage 
in  any  particular  mental  activity,  but  to  rest  as  completely  as 
possible.  The  work  used  in  this  experiment  during  the  variant 
interval  was  cross-multiplication,  the  multiplicand  and  the  mul- 
tiplier each  consisting  of  three  figures.  The  process  may  be 
best  given  by  an  example.  Take  736  x  284  ;  the  process  is  as 
follows  : 


4X6 

=  24 

4X3- 

h  8  X  6  +  2 

=  62 

4  X  7  H 

-2X6+8X3+ 

6  =  70 

8  X  7  H 

-3X2+7 

=  69 

2X7- 

h  6 

=  20 

Hence  the  answer  is  209024. 

The  subject  is  required  to  work  all  these  steps  mentally,  i.e., 
without  recourse  to  notes,  jotting  down  the  figures  "to  carry," 
or  similar  aids. 

The  test  follows  immediately  upon  the  variant  interval.  Be- 
tween the  closing  of  the  test  of  the  A  series  and  the  beginning 


A  STUDY  OF  RETROACTIVE  INHIBITION 


of  the  learning  of  the  B  series,  there  occurs,  as  noted  before, 
an  interval  of  at  least  five  minutes,  during  which  introspections 
and  remarks  of  the  subject  are  recorded.  During  this  interval 
the  subject  is  allowed  to  talk  freely  with  the  operator,  walk 
about  the  room,  etc.,  but  is  forbidden  to  do  any  thing  that  will 
act  as  an  unusual  excitant. 

The  subject  used  in  this  experiment  was  B.  He  was  ex- 
perienced in  learning  nonsense  syllables,  having  acted  as  a  sub- 
ject in  the  preliminary  experiments  leading  up  to  this  work. 
He  was  exceptionally  good  at  remembering  nonsense  syllables,  if 
allowed  to  form  sense  associations  between  them  when  learning. 
This  was  noted  in  the  preliminary  experiments,  and  he  was  in- 
structed not  to  form  such  associations.  We  may  say  that,  at  the 
beginning  of  the  present  experiment,  his  habit  of  forming  such 
sense  associations  was  fairly  well  broken. 

The  results  of  this  experiment  may  be  seen  from  the  following 
curves,  Fig.  2. 

A  and  B  are  success  curves.    C  is  the  time  curve  corresponding 


*       T     f      f 

FIG.  2 


i8  /.  EDGAR  DECAMP 

to  curve  A.  In  all  three  curves  the  abscissae  represent  the  num- 
ber of  minutes  of  the  variant  interval  employed  in  mental  ac- 
tivity. As  the  abscissae  increase  the  work  gradually  fills  the 
variant  interval  from  the  end  backwards.  E.g.,  under  abscissa 
6  the  experiments  would  be  disposed;  learning,  rest  nine  min- 
utes, work  six  minutes,  and  test.  In  curve  A  the  ordinates  repre- 
sent the  number  of  wholly  correct  responses  obtained  for  the 
different  abscissae.  In  B  the  ordinates  represent  the  wholly 
correct  responses  plus  the  partially  correct  responses  (two  letters 
correct  being  the  only  ones  considered),  each  of  these  latter 
evaluated  at  one-half — and  not  two-thirds — of  a  wholly  correct 
response.  In  curve  C  the  ordinates  represent  the  average  time, 
in  sigmata,  of  the  wholly  correct  responses  corresponding  to  the 
different  abscissae. 

There  were  a  few  correct  responses  for  which  the  operator, 
through  his  fault  or  some  defect  in  the  apparatus,  failed  to  get 
accurate  time.  Consequently,  in  the  computation  of  the  time 
curve  of  the  present  experiment,  as  well  as  of  those  of  subse- 
quent experiments,  any  ordinate  represents  the  average  time  for 
the  wholly  correct  responses  for  which  time  was  received. 

The  greatest  number  of  successes  was  obtained  with  rest  5, 
work  10;  the  least  number  with  rest  4,  work  n — two  adjacent 
points.  Their  separation  along  the  Y  axis,  is  greater  than  that 
between  any  other  two  points  of  the  curve.  One  is  hardly 
convinced  of  the  validity  of  such  a  disparity  existing  between 
adjacent  points.  No  essential  difference  between  such  points 
would  be  expected.  To  be  sure,  we  should  not  attempt  to  draw 
too  much  from  this  experiment,  because  of  the  small  number  of 
results  for  each  position  in  the  variant  interval.  If  we  consider 
the  success  curve  as  a  whole,  we  note  that  the  general  trend  is 
slightly  upward  to  point  10,  from  point  10  the  general  trend 
is  slightly  downward. 

Curve  B  shows  no  particular  value  over  curve  A.  Even  with 
so  few  results,  the  nature  of  the  two  curves  is  not  essentially 
different.  With  increasing  numbers  for  the  different  ordinates, 
it  appears  that  the  character  of  the  two  curves  would  be  less 
different.  Consequently,  it  scarcely  seems  necessary,  at  least 


A  STUDY  OF  RETROACTIVE  INHIBITION  19 

for  our  main  purpose,  to  consider  the  partial  responses  in  the 
computation  of  the  success  curve. 

If  we  consider  the  time  curve,  we  note,  starting  with  point 
10,  a  general  tendency  upwards.  Other  than  this  the  time  curve 
shows  no  particularly  clear  relation  to  the  success  curve. 

No  elaborate  introspections  were  taken  in  this  experiment. 
It  may  be  well  to  state  that  the  subject  repeatedly  indicated  that 
the  influence  of  the  interpolated  work  was  not  noticed,  asserting, 
that  if  he  had  a  syllable,  he  had  it,  and  the  work  had  no  noticeable 
effect. 

Relative  to  the  maximum  and  minimum  points  of  the  success 
curve,  it  may  be  remarked  that,  under  rest  4,  work  n,  one 
set  of  syllables,  erroneously  constructed,  was  given.  The  set 
yielded  three  correct  responses.  The  other  series  of  the  day, 
with  rest  n,  work  4,  yielded  no  correct  responses.  It  would 
seem  that  if  the  erroneous  construction — disobedience  of  re- 
striction No.  5 — had  any  influence,  it  favored  the  correct  re- 
sponses. If  we  allow  for  this,  it  would  mean  still  greater 
disparity  between  abscissae  10  and  n,  in  that  abscissa  n 
would  be  still  lower.  On  another  day,  with  similar  experimental 
conditions,  save  that  the  order  of  the  variant  interval  was,  in 
the  A  .series,  rest  n,  work  4,  and  in  the  B  series,  rest  4,  work 
1 1 ;  the  former  gave  three  correct  responses,  while  the  latter 
yielded  none.  The  subject  reported  that  he  thought  the  result, 
in  the  latter  case,  was  due,  not  to  the  work  introduced,  but  to 
the  difficulty  of  the  series  of  syllables.  If  we  consider  it  valid 
to  allow  for  this,  the  height  of  abscissa  n  would  be  raised. 
We  may  balance  this  allowance  against  the  previous  tendency. 

We  present  below  a  table  giving  in  outline  the  setting  and 
results  of  the  different  experiment  days  that  are  included  under 
Experiment  I. 


20  /.   EDGAR  DECAMP 


— i  /• — B.  Series — \ 

Sx         S3         S2         Sx         R.  W. 

o  15 

131                        i  14 

2  2  13 

23  I  3  12 

i  4  ii 

21  5  10 

3  69 

II  2  78 

4II87 
II  96 

i  i          10  5 

i  ii  4 

i  12  3 

41  13  2 

1  14  I 

2  15  O 

1  I  15  O 
32                             14  I 

2  13  2 

2  12  3 
14                                 I              II                  4 

31  10             5 

6  i  10             6 
3i  87 
3i  78 
S  69 

7  5  10 
i            4  ii 

131  3  12 

I  4  2  I  2  13 

3  i  14 
i                                  o  15 

10         76          15  8 

Under  E.  D.  we  have  the  temporal  order  of  any  particular 
experiment  day.  R.  and  W.  refer  to  the  composition  of  the 
variant  interval,  following  the  A  or  B  series,  as  the  case  may 
be.  Under  R.  we  have  the  number  of  minutes  rest  immediately 
following  the  learning  of  the  A  (or  B)  series  before  any  work 
is  engaged  in.  The  number  of  minutes  work  employed  is  found 
under  W.  In  the  columns  S3,  S2,  Si,  appears  the  absolute  number 
of  correct,  partially  correct  (two  letters  of  a  syllable),  and  par- 
tially correct  (one  letter  of  a  syllable),  responses  respectively. 
R.  plus  W.  should,  in  every  case,  equal  fifteen  minutes,  since 
fifteen  minutes  was  taken  for  the  length  of  the  variant  interval 
in  the  experiment.  However,  there  are  three  instances  in  which 
this  was  not  the  case.  In  neither  case  does  the  additional  minute 


i  —  A.  Series- 

E.D. 

R. 

W. 

S3 

S2 

i 

IS 

o 

i 

2 

2 

14 

i 

2 

I 

3 

13 

2 

4 

12 

3 

I 

2 

5 

II 

5 

3 

I 

6 

10 

5 

2 

7 

9 

6 

2 

I 

8 

8 

7 

9 

7 

8 

4 

10 

6 

9 

i 

I 

ii 

S 

IO 

3 

12 

4 

ii 

3 

I 

13 

3 

12 

2 

I 

14 

2 

13 

IS 

I 

14 

I 

I 

16 

0 

IS 

I 

17 

O 

15 

3 

18 

I 

14 

2 

I 

19 

2 

13 

I 

I 

20 

3 

12 

4 

21 

4 

II 

i 

22 

5 

IO 

3 

23 

6 

IO 

2 

24 

7 

8 

2 

I 

25 

8 

7 

3 

26 

9 

6 

2 

I 

27 

10 

5 

I 

28 

ii 

4 

3 

29 

12 

3 

3 

30 

13 

2 

2 

31 

14 

I 

7 

32 

IS 

O 

i 

2 

Totals 

66 

17 

A  STUDY  OF  RETROACTIVE  INHIBITION  21 

change  the  setting  until  after  nine  minutes  after  the  learning 
of  the  syllables.  The  writer  considers  the  point  negligible.  But, 
even  if  not  so,  the  results  of  the  particular  day  involved  weight 
against  (to  anticipate)  the  general  conclusion  of  this  paper. 

By  referring  to  the  table,  we  may  compare  the  total  successes 
of  the  A  series  with  the  total  successes  of  the  B  series.  We  find 
for  the  A  series,  2S3  =  66,  25.,=  17,  SS^io;  for  the  B  series, 
2S,  =  76,  2S2  =  15,  SS,  =  8.  The  advantage  in  favor  of  the 
B  series  is  a  trifle  greater  than  we  should  expect  chance  to  give 
either  series.  Due  to  the  mental  activity  connected  with  the  A 
series,  we  may  assume  a  slightly  fatigued  condition  of  the  sub- 
ject at  the  beginning  of  the  learning  of  the  B  series.  Hence, 
we  should  have  expected  the  A,  instead  of  the  B,  series  to  possess 
any  slight  advantage. 

Of  the  thirty-two  partially  correct  responses  (two  letters  cor- 
rect), twenty-two  were  of  the  Di-  type,  i.e.,  the  initial  consonant 
and  the  vowel  correct;  seven  were  of  the  -it  type,  i.e.,  the  vowel 
and  final  consonant  correct;  while  only  three  were  of  the  D-t 
type,  the  initial  and  final  consonants  correct.  This  was  to  be 
expected  since  chance,  in  a  combination  of  two  letters,  one  of 
which  is  a  vowel,  favors  a  consonant  and  a  vowel  rather  than 
two  consonants.  Further,  we  may  explain  the  advantage  of  the 
Di-  type  over  the  -it  type  in  accordance  with  the  laws  of  associa- 
tion. Clearly,  when  the  measure  Loc  Dit  is  learned,  the  associa- 
tion between  Loc  and  Di  is  much  stronger  than  that  between 
Loc  and  it. 

Of  the  eighteen  cases  of  partial  responses  (one  letter  correct), 
ten  of  these  letters  were  vowels,  and  eight  were  consonants. 
Of  the  eight  consonants,  four  were  initial  and  four  were  final. 
Here  again  we  should  expect  the  vowels,  relatively,  to  have  the 
advantage,  since  the  number  of  vowels  to  select  from  is  only  one- 
fourth  (approximately)  as  large  as  that  of  the  consonants. 
The  smallness  of  the  number  of  each  probably  renders  the  results 
more  or  less  meaningless. 

A  distribution  of  the  total  number  of  correct  responses  re- 
ceived, according  to  the  position  which  they  occupied  in  the 
learning,  shows  the  following : 


22  /.  EDGAR  DECAMP 

Position  .No.  of  Successes 

1  19 

2  2O 

3  18 

4  18 

5  20 

6  21 

7  26 

A  further  discussion  of  the  point  here  involved  will  be  deferred 
until  later. 

EXPERIMENT  II 

The  subject  for  this  experiment  was  F.  The  experiment  con- 
tinued sixty-four  experiment  days.  Fourteen-syllable  series  were 
used.  The  velocity  of  exposure  of  the  syllables  was  twenty-three 
and  one-third  seconds.  The  variant  interval  was  fifteen  minutes. 
The  work  used,  cross-multiplication.  In  fact,  the  experiment  is 
exactly  similar  to  Experiment  I,  with  two  exceptions,  (i)  the 
present  experiment  was  continued  longer. — to  sixty-four  days,  in- 
stead of  thirty-two;  and  (2)  a  different  subject  was  used.  Two 
complete  cycles  of  results  were  obtained,  giving  eight  results  for 
each  position  in  the  variant  interval.  As  nearly  as  possible,  the 
same  hour  of  the  day  on  different  experiment  days  was  used. 
A  curve  plotted  from  the  results  of  the  experiment  follows  in 
Figure  3. 

In  curve  A  the  abscissae  represent  the  number  of  minutes 
worked  for  the  different  positions  in  the  variant  interval.  The 
ordinates  represent  the  total  number  of  successes  for  any  posi- 
tion. In  curve  B  the  abscissae  are  the  same  as  in  curve  A. 
The  ordinates,  however,  represent  the  values  of  curve  A  plus 
the  partial  successes.  Only  those  responses  with  two  letters 
correct,  of  the  three  of  a  syllable,  are  considered,  and  each 
one  of  these  is  valued  at  one-half  the  value  of  a  successful  re- 
sponse. The  time  curve  is  shown  by  curve  C.  Abscissae  as  in 
A  and  B.  The  ordinates  represent  the  average  time  of  the 
correct  responses  for  any  position  in  the  variant  interval. 

The  following  points  may  be  noted : 

i.  The  lowest  point  of  the  success  curve  occurs  with  rest  i, 
work  14.  The  highest  point  occurs  with  the  position  rest 


A  STUDY  OF  RETROACTIVE  INHIBITION 


'0000 

f— 

1 

/000 

tooo 
•tfiooo 
fTooo 

-Jpoo 

icoo 
/.ooo 

/ 

\ 

/ 

\ 

/ 

\ 

/ 

\ 

X 

x 

\ 

\ 

/ 

1 

' 

\ 

> 

s 

f 

\ 

/ 

\ 

i 

So 

35 
3o 

20 

/a 

to 

£ 

^ 

X* 

\ 

/ 

i 

/ 

i 

X 

'/ 

\\ 

X 

-'' 

\ 

3 

\^ 

i  / 

' 

I 

> 

.  —  ' 

\ 

/ 

V 

.^, 

2 

\\ 

'7 

1 

I/ 

\ 

— 

' 

\ 

i 

/ 

*   J    <t    J 

r    *    7    rf 

FIG.  3 

^  /^  //  ^>  /j  /•»<  i 

2,  work  13.  These  two  points  are  adjacent,  and  it  is 
hard  to  account  for  such  a  difference  existing  between  two  points 
whose  position  would  seem,  naturally,  to  merit  no  such  dis- 
tinction. We  noted  a  similar  occurrence  in  Experiment  I,  only  in 
that  experiment  the  divergent  points  were  n  and  12  instead 
of,  as  in  the  present  experiment,  13  and  14. 

2.  To  the  lowest  point  of  the  success  curve  corresponds  the 
lowest  point  of  the  time  curve,  but  to  the  two  next  lowest  points 
of  the  success  curve  correspond  the  two  highest  points  of  the 
time  curve — two  results  that  do  not  seemingly  harmonize  with 
themselves,  and  with  what  we  might  have  expected.  It  would 
seem  reasonable  to  suppose,  from  our  general  idea  of  retroactive 
inhibition,  that  with  a  large  number  of  successes  would  corre- 
spond short  times.  But  this  is  not  the  case  in  our  curves.  In 
fact,  there  seems  to  be  no  simple  expressible  relation  between  the 


24  /.  EDGAR  DECAMP 

two  curves.  According  to  the  results  of  Miiller  and  Pilzecker 
this  relation  is  an  inverse  one. 

3.  The  undulatory  character  of  the  success  curve,  noticeable 
also  in  the  success  curve  of  Experiment  I,  is  no  less  surprising 
than  bewildering. 

The  writer  wishes  to  call  attention  to  the  fact  that  the  subject 
used  in  this  experiment  had  had  considerable  experience  in 
learning  nonsense  syllables  previous  to  this  experiment.  Again, 
he  did  not  have  such  a  tendency  to  form  sense  associations  as 
was  the  case  with  the  subject  in  Experiment  I.  It  may  be  well  to 
mention  here  that  subject  F.  and  subject  B.  were  quite  different 
in  temperament;  subject  F.  being  calm,  deliberate,  slow  and  easy 
going;  while  subject  B.  was  quite  the  opposite,  quick,  active  and 
alert. 

Here,  as  in  Experiment  I,  a  large  number  of  introspections  is 
wanting.  The  subject  reported  no  noticeable  effect  of  the  inter- 
polated work.  He  reported  that  he  though  the  process  of  mul- 
tiplication tended  to  become  a  mechanical  one,  not  requiring  the 
closest  attention.  He  had  at  one  time  held  a  clerical  position 
requiring  the  constant  use  of  figures.  His  idea  was  that  such 
work  may  become  automatic,  columns  of  figures  may  be  added 
while  the  attention  is  directed  elsewhere.  The  writer  has  acted 
as  subject  where  cross  multiplication  was  used;  he  is  also  accus- 
tomed to  the  frequent  use  of  figures  in  clerical  work.  With  him 
the  process  in  the  cases  cited  seems  to  be  far  removed  from  the 
mechanical.  To  be  sure,  after  a  little  practice,  the  work  can 
be  done  with  greater  rapidity ;  the  figures  seem  to  fall  into  line, 
as  it  were ;  the  result  seems  to  come  forthwith,  but  most  success- 
fully, only  when  the  attention  is  closely  given  to  the  work  in 
hand.1 

A  table  follows  showing  the  setting  and  results  of  each  day's 
experiment : 

1  Cf.  Ladd  and  Woodworth's  position  relative  to  habitual  actions,  Elements 
of  Physiological  Psychology,  1911,  p.  564. 


TABLE  II— A   Series 

E.D.  D--    Di-  -i-        -it      - -t 

1  i 

2  i  i 

3  i         i 

4  i  i 

5  2  i 

6  i        i 

7  i  i 

8  i  i 

9  i 

10  i 

11  i 

12  I      I 

13  3 

14  i 
15 

16 

17  i        i 

18  i        i 

19  i 

20  ii 

21  I 

22  II 

23  211 

24  I 

25  I      I 

26  I 

27  I 

28  I 


1  I 

I 
I 

I 

2  I 


I 

2     I 
I 
I 

I 


1  2 

2  I      I 
I 

I  I 

2 

I 
I 


38    28    20 


D-t 

R. 

W. 

S3 

S2 

§, 

IS 

o 

3 

i 

14 

i 

5 

i 

i 

13 

2 

3 

2 

12 

3 

5 

r 

I 

I 

II 

4 

i 

3 

I 

IO 

5 

2 

2 

9 

6 

3 

I 

I 

8 

7 

3 

I 

I 

7 

8 

3 

I 

6 

9 

4 

I 

5 

IO 

2 

I 

4 

ii 

2 

I 

I 

I 

3 

12 

3 

4 

2 

13 

5 

i 

I 

I 

14 

ii 

0 

15 

4 

O 

15 

5 

2 

I 

14 

3 

2 

I 

2 

13 

5 

I 

3 

12 

5 

2 

4 

II 

4 

I 

5 

10 

2 

I 

I 

6 

9 

I 

2 

2 

7 

8 

6 

I 

8 

7 

3 

I 

I 

9 

6 

4 

I 

10 

5 

5 

I 

ii 

4 

5 

I 

12 

3 

7 

13 

2 

5 

14 

I 

4 

I 

I 

I 

IS 

O 

4 

I 

I 

O 

15 

4 

2 

14 

I 

6 

I 

13 

2 

4 

2 

I 

II 

4 

4 

I 

2 

12 

3 

5 

2 

IO 

5 

4 

I 

9 

6 

4 

I 

I 

I 

8 

7 

2 

2 

7 

8 

4 

6 

9 

6 

5 

10 

i 

I 

2 

4 

ii 

2 

2 

3 

12 

4 

I 

I 

2 

13 

4 

I 

I 

I 

14 

I 

O 

15 

5 

I 

15 

o 

3 

I 

2 

I 

14 

5 

I 

2 

13 

6 

I 

3 

12 

5 

I 

I 

4 

II 

4 

I 

5 

IO 

4 

I 

6 

9 

5 

7 

8 

i 

I 

2 

8 

7 

i 

3 

I 

9 

6 

6 

i 

IO 

5 

3 

2 

V 

ii 

4 

3 

2 

12 

3 

4 

I 

I 

13 

2 

4 

2 

14 

I 

4 

I 

I 

15 

6 

8 

239 

66 

38 

TABLE  II— B  Series 

D-t 


E.D. 

S3 

S2 

s, 

R. 

W. 

D-  -  Di- 

-i- 

-it 

--t 

i 

2 

2 

I 

0 

15 

i 

i 

I 

2 

4 

i 

14 

3 

3 

I 

I 

2 

13 

i 

i 

4 

i 

5 

I 

3 

12 

2 

i 

3 

5 

2 

i 

2 

4 

II 

I 

2 

6 

I 

i 

2 

5 

IO 

I 

I 

i 

7 

3 

2 

6 

9 

2 

8 

2 

I 

I 

9 

6 

I      I 

9 

3 

8 

7 

I 

2 

10 

I 

I 

I 

7 

8 

I 

I 

ii 

3 

2 

IO 

5 

2 

12 

2 

I 

I 

ii 

4 

I      I 

13 

2 

I 

12 

3 

I 

14 

I 

3 

13 

2 

3 

15 

I 

2 

14 

I 

2 

16 

3 

I 

15 

O 

I 

17 

I 

5 

15 

O 

2 

3 

18 

3 

I 

i 

14 

I 

I 

i 

19 

4 

2 

i 

13 

2 

i 

2 

20 

7 

12 

3 

21 

i 

I 

2 

II 

4 

I 

i 

I 

22 

i 

2 

IO 

5 

2 

23 

3 

I 

I 

9 

6 

I 

i 

24 

6 

I 

8 

7 

I 

25 

3 

2 

7 

8 

I 

26 

6 

6 

9 

27 

6 

5 

IO 

28 

3 

2 

4 

ii 

2 

29 

5 

I 

3' 

12 

I 

30 

5 

I 

2 

13 

i 

31 

i 

2 

2 

I 

14 

I 

2 

32 

4 

2 

0 

15 

2 

33 

5 

2 

15 

o 

2 

34 

3 

I 

I 

i 

14 

I 

I 

35 

7 

2 

13 

36 

2 

I 

3 

4 

ii 

2     I 

I 

37 

5 

3 

12 

38 

i 

I 

3 

5 

IO 

3 

I 

39 

4 

6 

9 

40 

2 

4 

7 

8 

4 

2 

41 

4 

i 

8 

7 

i 

42 

4 

i 

9 

6 

I 

43 

4 

I 

10 

5 

I 

44 

5 

i 

ii 

4 

i 

45 

2 

i 

12 

3 

I 

46 

5 

I 

13 

2 

47 

4 

I 

14 

I 

I 

48 

3 

I 

15 

O 

I 

49 

3 

3 

i 

o 

IS 

I 

i 

2 

50 

2 

2 

i 

14 

i 

I      I 

I 

51 

7 

13 

2 

52 

4 

I 

i 

12 

3 

I      I 

53 

4 

I 

i 

II 

4 

I 

i 

54 

5 

I 

i 

IO 

5 

i 

I 

55 

6 

9 

6 

56 

3 

2 

i 

8 

7 

2 

i 

57 

4 

3 

7 

8 

2 

I 

58 

4 

2 

6 

9 

2 

59 

4 

5 

IO 

60 

2 

2 

4 

ii 

I 

61 

4 

I 

3 

12 

I 

62 

3 

i 

2 

13 

I 

63 

3 

3 

I 

14 

3 

64 

4 

2 

O 

15 

2 

2IO 

6l 

62 

II    40 

43 

17 

8 

A  STUDY  OF  RETROACTIVE  INHIBITION  27 

The  significance  of  the  different  abbreviations  is  fully  explained 
under  Table  I,  Experiment  I,  page  20. 

A  point,  scarcely  worthy  of  note,  is  that  the  temporal  order 
of  the  experiments  on  the  eighth,  ninth,  and  tenth  days,  is  the  re- 
verse of  our  plan.  The  same  occurs  with  days  36  and  37.  The 
A  and  B  series  of  days  33  and  49  are  the  reverse  of  the  planned 
order.  Both  days,  however,  correspond  in  setting,  hence  the 
reversal  of  one  practically  equalizes  the  reversal  of  the  other. 

In  our  present  results,  of  the  total  successes  for  the  A  and  B 
series,  we  note  quite  the  opposite  to  that  received  in  Experiment  I. 
The  A  series  gives,  on  the  whole,  283=239,  2S2=66,  2SX=  38 ; 
while  the  B  series  gives  2S3  =  210,  2S,  =  6i,  2S  l=  62.  We 
may  explain  our  present  result  in  accordance  with  our  previous 
reasoning,  (p.  21),  that  the  second  set  of  the  day  may  be  at 
a  slight  disadvantage.  This,  together  with  chance  variation,  we 
may  say,  gives  rise  to  the  difference  existing  between  2S3  of  A 
and  2S3  of  B. 

We  wish  to  mention  another  point,  the  details  of  which  are 
not  presented.  If  we  proportion  the  total  449  (  2S3  (A)  plus 
23S  (B))  correct  responses  according  to  their  temporal  order, 
as  respects  measures,  in  the  learning,  we  obtain: 

Position  Responses 

1  74 

2  Si 

3  60 

4  62 

5  66 

6  63 

7  72 

It  was  very  noticeable  with  subject  F.  that  he  often  knew  the 
position  a  syllable  occupied  in  the  learning  when  he  could  not 
recall  that  syllable.  It  is  to  be  remembered  that  the  order  of 
the  syllables  in  the  test  was  not  the  same  as  that  in  the  learning, 
and  the  order  for  the  test  of  the  A  series  was  not  the  same 
as  that  for  the  B  series. 

The  subject  remarked  that  the  syllable  learned  first  often  fails 
to  come  up  in  the  recall.  We  could  attribute  this  to  an  assurance 
at  the  first  causing  a  comparative  lack  of  attention  to  that  syllable 
in  the  latter  part  of  the  learning  process.  The  subject  also  noted 


28  7.  EDGAR  DECAMP 

that  he  thought  the  getting  of  the  response  to  the  first  test 
syllable  had  a  particular  influence  upon  the  total  successes  of 
that  trial.  His  idea  being,  caeteris  paribus,  a  successful  re- 
sponse to  the  first  test  syllable  initiates  a  kind  of  systematic  pre- 
paration for  the  responses  to  the  remaining  test  syllables. 
Theoretically,  this  appears  plausible;  if  we  assume  the  subject 
has  a  certain  stock  of  correct  responses,  consciously  or  uncon- 
sciously, at  the  beginning  of  the  test,  the  precipitation  of  any 
one  of  them  may  prepare  the  remaining  and  lend  a  certain  amount 
of  impetus,  due  to  the  feeling  tone  connected  with  the  successful 
response.  This  effect,  coming  at  the  beginning,  is  much  more 
potent,  since  the  succeeding  test  syllables  are  unimpaired  by  any 
unsuccessful  inhibiting  attitude,  which  is  necessarily  the  case 
when  the  first  or  first  and  second  responses  are  unsuccessful. 

The  subject's  idea  of  how  he  gets  the  correct  responses  may 
be  instructive.  They  are  due  to  one,  or  some  combination  of, 
the  three  factors,  (i)  direct  association,  (2)  getting  the  position 
occupied  by  the  syllable  in  the  learning,  and  (3)  running  over  the 
stock  of  syllables  on  hand.  Towards  the  end  of  the  experiment 
the  subject  indicated  that  there  was  much  more  to  learning  a 
set  of  syllables  than  the  mere  allowing  them  to  pass  before  the 
field  of  vision,  framing  his  view,  "Some  force  is  let  loose  that 
feels  like  an  original  will-power" ;  meaning  probably  nothing 
more  than  a  concentration  of  attention,  or,  as  expressed  by 
Ladd  and  Woodworth2  "setting  up  a  favorable  adjustment." 

EXPERIMENT  III 

The  two  previous  experiments  have  partially  indicated  the 
difficulty  of  our  task.  It  is  to  be  remembered  that  the  present- 
work  was  intended  to  be  an  elaboration  of  the  work  done  by 
Miiller  and  Pilzecker  on  rectroactive  inhibition.  Consequently, 
we  assumed  their  work  as  a  base.  Neither  our  preliminary  ex- 
periments nor  the  two  experiments  given  above,  showed  any 
evidence  that  we  were  going  to  receive  any  such  "striking" 
results  as  those  received  by  the  German  investigators.  Naturally, 
it  appeared  necessary  to  conduct  experiments,  similar  to  the  ones 

2  Elements  of  Physiological  Psychology,  1911,  p.  582. 


A  STUDY  OF  RETROACTIVE  INHIBITION  29 

carried  out  by  them,  for  the  purpose  of  confirming  their  results, 
or  establishing  new  ones. 

The  general  method  of  this  experiment  was  similar  to  that 
of  Experiments  I  and  II,  but  different  from  them  in  that,  in  our 
present  experiment,  only  two  points  of  the  variant  interval  are 
under  investigation.  On  each  experiment  day  two  series — an 
A  and  a  B — of  seven  measures,  two  syllables  to  the  measure, 
were  learned.  Ten  repetitions  were  used  for  each  series.  Sub- 
ject B.,  the  same  as  in  Experiment  I.  Following  the  learning 
of  one  series,  there  was  a  rest  period  of  fifteen  minutes,  then  the 
test.  Immediately  following  the  learning  of  the  other  series, 
the  subject  engaged  in  cross  multiplication  for  ten  minutes.  A 
rest  period  of  five  minutes  followed  the  multiplication,  then 
the  test  was  made.  On  one  day  the  multiplication  was  placed  in 
the  A  series,  on  the  next  day  it  was  placed  in  the  B  series.  This 
gives  us  an  equal  distribution  between  the  A  and  B  series  of 
the  deleterious  effect  of  any  fatigue,  arising  from  having  learned 
a  previous  set  of  syllables.  Of  course,  even  then,  the  objection 
lies  open  that,  from  the  standpoint  of  fatigue,  the  set  with  work 
following  has  a  slight  advantage  in  that,  when  it  occupies  the 
second  position  (B  series),  no  work  was  done  during  the  pre- 
ceding variant  interval.  When  the  set,  with  no  work  following, 
occupied  the  second  position,  ten  minutes  of  work  were  engaged 
in  during  the  variant  interval  of  the  first  set. 

The  experiment  extended  over  a  period  of  1 6  experiment  days 
with  the  following  results,  n  =  112: 

TABLE  III 

S3     r     m.v.  %  S2  Sj  Tr     T<2OOO(r 

Cs.  I.     Rest  15   54    3.4    1.6    48  5    4  2221            31           22 

Cs.  II.    Work  10,  Rest  5 37    2.3     i.o    33  4    4  2673            15              9 

The  above  table  gives  the  absolute  number  of  successes,  S3; 
the  average  number  in  a  set,  r ;  the  per  centum,  % ;  the  mean 
variation,  m.v.;  the  number  of  syllables,  two  letters  correct,  S2; 
the  number  of  syllables,  one  letter  correct,  Sij  the  average  time 
for  the  correct  responses,  Tr;  the  absolute  number  of  correct 
responses  with  time  less  than  2000  sigmata,  T  <  20000-;  and 
tlie  number  with  time  less  than  1,500  sigmata,  T  <  15000-. 


30  7.  EDGAR  DECAMP 

The  results  obtained  in  this  experiment  seem  to  favor  the 
assumption  that  retroactive  inhibition  plays  a  part  under  such 
conditions  as  those  of  Case  II.  However,  the  force  of  these 
results  becomes  somewhat  weakened,  when  viewed  in  connection 
with  those  received  in  the  next  experiment  with  the  same  subject. 

EXPERIMENT  IV 

The  present  experiment  extended  over  a  period  of  ten  ex- 
periment days.  Subject,  B.  Seven-measure  nonsense  syllables 
were  repeated  ten  times.  The  variant  interval  of  fifteen  minutes 
was  thus  disposed.  On  each  experiment  day  two  series  were 
learned.  Immediately  following  one  series  there  elapsed  a  rest 
period  of  six  minutes,  the  remaining  nine  minutes  were  occupied 
in  cross-multiplication.  Following  the  other  set  there  elapsed 
a  rest  period  of  two  minutes,  then  thirteen  minutes  of  cross- 
multiplication.  At  the  close  of  the  variant  interval,  the  syllables 
were  tested.  A  rest  period  of  at  least  five  minutes  occurred  be- 
tween the  test  of  the  first  and  the  learning  of  the  second  series 
on  any  experiment  day.  On  one  experiment  day  the  variant 
interval,  containing  the  rest  six  minutes,  work  nine  minutes,  fol- 
lowed the  learning  of  the  A  series;  on  the  next  day  it  followed 
the  learning  of  the  B  series.  The  results  follow  in  Table  IV, 
n  =  70: 


S8        r      tn.v.     %      S2    St      Tr        T<2OOO<r 

Cs.     I.    Rest  6,  Work    9 20      2         i         29      3      2      2675  14 

Cs.  II.    Rest  2,  Work  13 26      2.6      1.6      37      2      o      3328  n 

Above  is  given,  in  tabular  form,  the  absolute  number  of  suc- 
cesses, S3 ;  the  average  number,  r ;  the  mean  variation,  m.  v. ; 
the  per  centum,  % ;  the  number  of  syllables,  two  letters  correct, 
S2;  the  number  of  syllables,  one  letter  correct,  Sij  the  average 
time  for  the  correct  responses,  Tr;  and  the  absolute  number  of 
correct  responses  with  time  less  than  2000  sigmata,  T  <  200000-. 

Attention  may  be  called  to  the  fact  that  Case  II,  though  its 
variant  interval  contains  more  work,  and  given  sooner  after 
learning,  than  Case  I,  gives  more  successful  responses.  The 


A  STUDY  OF  RETROACTIVE  INHIBITION  31 

average  time  for  Case  II  is  longer  than  that  for  Case  I.  There 
appears  no  evident  reason  for  the  getting  of  more  successes  in 
Case  II.  The  suggestion  offers  itself  that  the  greater  mental 
activity  of  Case  II  placed  the  subject  in  a  condition  of  better 
mental  preparedness  for  the  test  than  did  Case  I.  Assuming 
the  activity  of  retroactive  inhibition,  the  time  result  is  what  we 
should  expect. 

The  results  of  this  experiment  suggest  that  two  minutes  after 
the  learning  of  a  series,  retroactive  inhibition  plays  no  notice- 
able part  in  diminishing  the  number  of  successful  responses, 
though  it  may  appear  in  a  slight  retardation  of  the  correct  re- 
sponses. The  author  realizes  that  the  meagreness  of  numbers, 
if  we  consider  in  isolation  the  experiment  at  hand,  is  a  serious 
drawback  to  final  acceptance  of  any  apparent  conclusions 
therefrom. 

EXPERIMENT  V 

This  experiment  was  conducted  exactly  as  Experiment  IV,  save 
with  a  different  subject,  subject  D.,  and  with  an  essential  change 
with  respect  to  the  variant  interval.  In  the  present  experiment, 
the  variant  interval  of  fifteen  minutes  was  thus  occupied;  the 
subject  either  cross-multiplied  the  full  time,  beginning  im- 
mediately after  learning,  or  he  rested  the  full  time.  On  one 
day  the  cross-multiplication  came  after  the  A  series,  on  the 
next  day  it  came  after  the  B  series.  The  subject  was  an  ex- 
perienced one  in  learning  nonsense  syllables.  He  tended  rather 
toward  the  steady,  slow,  deliberate  temperament.  The  experi- 
ment continued  for  ten  experiment  days,  with  the  following 
results ;  Table  V,  n  =  70 : 

TABtLE  V 

S3        r        m.v.    %  S2  St  Tr  T<2Oocw 

Cs.  I.    Rest  15 24      2.4        .96      34  4  9  5149  6 

Cs.  II.    Work  15 21      2.1       1.31      30  5  5  5822  2 

The  column  symbols  have  exactly  the  same  meaning  as  those 
used  in  Experiments  III  and  IV,  p.  29$. 

Case  I  shows  a  slight  advantage  over  Case  II,  with  respect 
to  the  number  of  successful  responses.  A  slight  advantage  is 


32  /.  EDGAR  DECAMP 

all  that  can  be  claimed;  even  that — it  is  not  unreasonable  to 
assume — may  have  been  due  to  mere  chance.  To  be  sure,  the 
argument  works  both  ways  equally  well;  it  may  be  claimed  that 
chance  favored  the  results  of  Case  II. 

Surely,  if  we  are  to  obtain  a  retroactive  effect,  it  should  have 
appeared  in  Case  II,  with  fifteen  minutes  work  immediately  sub- 
sequent upon  the  learning.  Shall  we  then  tend  towards  some 
such  conclusion  as  the  non-existence  of  retroactive  inhibition? 
Or,  shall  we  refrain — doubtless  the  better  way — on  account  of 
the  paucity  of  results,  from  drawing  any  general  conclusions, 
until  the  question  is  more  fully  investigated?  It  seems  that  we 
may  be  allowed  to  question  the  importance  of  the  part  retro- 
active inhibition  has  hitherto  been  assumed  to  play  in  influencing 
the  recall  of  nonsense  syllables. 

EXPERIMENT  VI 

The  plan  of  this  experiment  was  the  same  as  that  pursued 
in  the  preceding  experiment,  save  that  in  this  a  different  sub- 
ject, subject  W.,  was  used,  one  who  had  not  had  very  much 
experience  in  learning  nonsense  syllables.  Ten  repetitions 
were  given  each  set  of  seven-measure  syllables.  The  variant  in- 
tervals were  of  two  kinds,  one  where  the  subject  rested  the  full 
fifteen  minutes;  the  other  where  immediately  after  the  learning, 
the  subject  worked  at  cross-multiplication  for  ten  minutes,  and 
then  rested  the  remaining  five  minutes.  The  test  followed  the 
variant  interval.  The  experiment  continued  nine  experiment 
days.  The  results  obtained  are  given  below  in  Table  VI,  n  =  63  : 

TABLE  VI 

S3        r      m.v.     %       S2     S,      Tr        T<2OOO<r 

Cs.    I.     Rest  15 50      5.6      1.2      79      5      o      2828  18 

Cs.  II.    Work  10,  Rest  5 43      4.8      1.3      68      2      i      2955  12 

The  symbols  at  the  head  of  the  different  columns  have  been 
fully  explained.  See  above  under  Experiments  III  and  IV,  p.  2()f. 

We  note  here  an  advantage  in  favor  of  Case  I,  in  respect  both 
to  the  number  of  correct  responses,  and  the  celerity  of  the  re- 
sponse. The  advantage,  however,  is  not  a  decided  one ;  especially 
is  this  the  case  with  respect  to  time.  It  is  noticeable  that  Case  I 


A  STUDY  OF  RETROACTIVE  INHIBITION 


33 


shows  more  correct  responses  with  reaction  time  less  than  2000 
sigmata.  The  advantage  in  favor  of  Case  I,  on  the  part  of  the 
successful  responses,  seems  too  large  to  be  wholly  accounted 
for  on  the  basis  of  variation  and  chance. 

EXPERIMENT  VII 

W.  acted  as  subject  for  the  present  experiment.  The  only 
essential  difference  between  the  setting  of  this  experiment  and 
that  of  Experiment  VI,  lies  in  the  fact,  that,  in  the  learning, 
the  syllables  were  presented  auditorially  instead  of  visually. 
This  was  accomplished  by  reading  the  syllables  to  the  subject. 
Six  repetitions  were  given  each  series.  The  metronome  was  set 
at  72.  The  operator  began  a  measure  on  one  stroke  of  the 
metronome  and  ended  on  the  next;  two  strokes  were  allowed 
for  rest ;  then  a  new  measure  was  read.  For  the  test  additional 
apparatus  was  used,  a  second  mouth-key,  a  lamp  battery  and  a 
relay.  It  will  be  recalled  that  the  mouth-key  is  fitted  with 
tambour  and  metal  pointer  for  breaking  a  circuit  only.  With 
the  use  of  two  circuit  breakers  alone,  the  use  of  the  chronoscope 
is  impractical  either  with  open  or  closed  circuit.  The  difficulty 
is  obviated  by  the  use  of  the  lamp  battery  and  the  relay  apparatus. 
The  set-up  may  be  shown  diagrammatically  as  follows,  Fig.  4: 


FIG.  4 


34  /.   EDGAR  DECAMP 

M.  K.  I,  mouth-key  for  the  subject;  C,  commutator;  L.  B., 
lamp  battery;  H.  C,  Hipp  chronoscope;  R.,  relay;  and  M.  K.  2, 
mouth-key  for  the  operator. 

The  complete  apparatus  was  used  only  in  the  test.  The  pur- 
pose of  the  lamp  battery  and  relay  was  to  give  a  closed  circuit 
through  the  chronoscope,  when  the  operator  spoke  the  test 
syllable  into  M.  K.  2.  The  subject's  response  was  spoken  into 
M.  K.  i.  A  rubber  tube  led  out  from  M.  K.  2  to  the  subject's 
ear.  The  subject's  end  of  the  tube  was  fitted  with  a  common 
phonographic  listening  device.  This  device  consists  of  a  con- 
cavo-convex disk  (diam.  5  cm.)  of  wood,  through  the  centre 
of  which  runs  a  short  piece  of  tin  tubing. 

The  experiment  continued  ten  experiment  days  with  the  fol- 
lowing results,  Table  VII,  n  =  70: 

TABLE  VII 

S3      r    m.v.    %     S2  S,  Tr      T<2OOO<r  T<3OOO 

Cs.    I.    Rest  15   28    2.9    i.i    41     ii  6  5127        7                  11 

Cs.  II.    Work  10,  Rest  5.. 28    2.8    0.7    40    15  4  4286       4                  11 

The  meaning  of  the  headings  of  the  different  columns  is 
fully  explained  under  Experiments  III  and  IV,  p.  2Qf.,  save  that 
we  have  added  a  column,  T  <  30000-,  which  includes  the  absolute 
number  of  successes  with  time  less  than  3000  sigmata.  It  should 
be  mentioned  that  the  measure  of  correctness  of  a  response  con- 
sisted in  its  agreement  with  the  correct  syllable  in  sound,  and 
not  in  spelling,  as  in  the  case  of  those  syllables  given  visually. 

We  note  here,  with  respect  to  the  number  of  successful  re- 
sponses, practically  no  particular  advantage  shown  by  either  case. 
An  exceptional  result  occurs  in  that  the  average  time  for  the 
correct  responses  of  Case  II  is  less  than  that  for  the  correct 
responses  of  Case  I.  In  our  previous  experiments,  the  average 
time  for  Case  II  has  always  been  greater  than  that  for  Case  I. 
Case  I  shows  an  advantage  in  the  number  of  very  short  times, 
less  than  2000  sigmata,  seven  as  compared  with  four  for  Case  I, 
but  when  3000  sigmata  is  the  maximum  time  for  comparison, 
both  cases  yield  the  same  number  of  times  less  than  that  amount. 


A  STUDY  OF  RETROACTIVE  INHIBITION  35 

EXPERIMENT  VIII 

The  general  method  of  this  experiment  was  the  same  as  that 
used  in  Experiment  VI.  D.  acted  as  subject;  fourteen-syllable 
nonsense  series  were  used.  Two  sets  were  learned  on  each  day. 
A  variant  interval  of  sixteen  minutes  was  thus  disposed :  In- 
stead of  using  cross-multiplication  as  interpolated  work,  as  in 
the  previous  experiments,  physical  work  was  employed.  The 
subject  was  harnessed  to  an  ergograph,  so  that  the  weight  would 
be  lifted  from  the  shoulders.  The  lifts  were  made  with  the 
metronome,  one  every  two  seconds.  Immediately  after  the  learn- 
ing of  one  set  of  syllables,  one  minute  was  taken  for  adjustment 
of  the  apparatus  to  the  subject,  then  came  ten  minutes  work  with 
the  ergograph,  followed  by  a  rest  of  five  minutes,  then  the  test. 
In  a  second  case  no  work  followed  the  learning  of  a  series  of 
syllables.  On  one  day  the  work  came  in  the  A  series,  on  the 
next  day  it  came  in  the  B  series.  The  experiment  continued 
nine  experiment  days,  giving  the  following  results,  Table  VIII, 
11=63: 

TABLE  VIII 

S3      r    m.v.    %  S2  S,  Tr    T<2OOOcr  T<30OO«r 

Cs.  I.    Rest  16 42      4.7       1.2      67  4  4  4862  13  19 

Cs.  II.    Adj.  i,  Erg.  10,  Rest  5 27      3.0      0.9      43  2  2  4108  8  14 

For  explanation  of  symbols  used  see  Experiments  III,  IV  and 
VII. 

Here  we  have  quite  an  advantage  in  favor  of  Case  I.  The 
number  of  successes  afforded  by  Case  II  is  open  to  the  very 
serious  objection  that  its  smallness  may  be  due  to  physical  fatigue 
rather  than  retroactive  inhibition.  The  difference  is  quite  ex- 
plainable upon  the  basis  that,  at  the  time  of  the  test,  the  lingering 
fatigue  sensations,  together  with  the  associated  complex  of  "sen- 
sations of  rest,"  prevented  complete  attention  to  the  recall  of  the 
learned  nonsense  syllables.  This,  it  appears  to  the  writer,  is  a 
sufficient  explanation  of  the  existing  difference,  without  necessi- 
tating the  employment  of  a  shock  effect  of  the  physical  work 
upon  the  learned  syllables.  In  case  the  shock  effect  existed,  the 
present  experiment  was  not  adequately  designed  to  differentiate 
it  from  the  fatigue  effect. 


36  /.  EDGAR  DE  CAMP 

EXPERIMENT  IX  (a) 

Hitherto,  in  our  discussion,  we  have  been  hampered  and  forced 
to  refrain  from  any  final  conclusions  because  of  the  paucity  of 
numbers.  Hence  it  was  desired  to  conduct  an  experiment  to 
such  length  that  the  weight  of  numbers  should  not  be  wanting. 
It  was  the  experimenter's  purpose,  in  the  present  experiment, 
to  meet  such  a  requirement.  In  order  that  the  work  might  not 
become  too  laborious,  and  extend  to  too  great  length,  fewer 
points  of  the  variant  interval  were  taken  for  investigation  than 
were  taken  in  Experiments  I  and  II,  whose  general  plan  the 
present  experiment  follows.  The  length  of  the  variant  interval 
was  fifteen  minutes.  The  first  six  minutes  of  the  variant  in- 
terval were  the  only  ones  subjected  to  variance,  the  other  nine 
minutes  serving  as  a  rest  period  previous  to  the  test.  In  justi- 
fication of  so  narrowly  restricting  the  variant  part,  we  may  cite 
experiments  I  to  VII.  From  them  the  only  conclusion  relative 
to  retroactive  inhibition,  we  wish  to  urge,  is  that  mental  activity, 
following  three  or  four  minutes  after  the  learning  of  a  set  of 
nonsense  syllables,  produces  no  apparent  retroactive  effect  upon 
the  number  of  successful  responses  in  the  recall.  Consequently, 
we  consider  the  limits  of  the  variant  factor  in  the  present  ex- 
periment as  sufficient,  and  deem  more  extended  limits  unnecessary 
in  the  investigation  of  the  factor  under  consideration. 

Seven  measures,  two  syllables  to  the  measure,  were  repeated 
ten  times  by  the  subject.  The  first  six  minutes  following  the 
learning  were  disposed  in  accordance  with  the  day's  position 
in  the  cyclic  order.  In  consequence  of  the  length  of  the  variant 
part  of  the  variant  interval,  the  cycle,  which  is  fully  explained 
supra,  (p.  isf.),  consists  of  fourteen  experiment  days.  Follow- 
ing the  variant  interval  of  fifteen  minutes  came  the  test.  Here 
the  experiment  days  follow  fairly  closely  the  calendar  days.  In 
all  instances  the  work  is  carried  out  in  the  afternoon,  and  for  the 
most  part  at  the  same  hour  of  the  day. 

Z.  B.  served  as  subject.  The  experiment  continued  seventy 
days,  thus  completing  five  cycles,  and  giving  twenty  different  re- 
sults for  each  point  in  the  variant  part  of  the  variant  interval. 
A  success  curve,  A,  and  a  time  curve,  B,  plotted  from  the  results, 
follow  in  Figure  5,  n  =  140: 


Sooo 


tooo 


Sttoo 


3  OOO 


i  000 


/ooo 


V 

\ 

^ 

• 

V 

\ 

/ 

' 

\ 

A_ 

JO 
A 

t      z      3      ¥•    JT     6 

FIG.  5 


38  /.  EDGAR  DECAMP 

A  is  a  curve  representing  the  total  successes.  B  represents 
the  time  curve.  In  A  and  B  the  abscissae  represent  the  number 
of  minutes  in  which  mental  activity  (cross-multiplication)  was 
employed  in  the  variant  part  of  the  variant  interval;  e.  g., 
abscissa  4  corresponds  to  the  day's  arrangement,  learning, 
rest  two  minutes,  work  four  minutes,  rest  nine  minutes,  and  test. 
In  A  the  ordinates  represent  the  relative  number  of  successful 
responses.  In  B  the  ordinates  represent  the  average  time  of 
the  successful  responses. 

A  subsidiary  curve,  plotted  in  accordance  with  the  method 
used  in  plotting  Curve  B,  Fig.  2,  p.  i/f.  and  Curve  B.  Fig.  3, 
p.  22 f.,  shows  no  essential  difference  from  our  present  Curve  A, 
the  courses  of  the  two  curves  always  running  in  the  same 
direction.  Consequently  this  curve  has  been  omitted. 

The  highest  ordinate  of  the  success  curve  occurs  under  abscissa 
o,  where  no  work  occurred  in  the  variant  interval.  A  seemingly 
non-harmonizing  result  occurs  in  that  abscissa  i  vies  with 
abscissa  6  for  the  lowest  point  of  the  curve.  In  order  to 
consider  the  results  more  in  detail  two  tables,  Table  IX  and 
Table  X,  are  subjoined. 

TABLE  IX 

Minutes    Worked 


1  

-c  — 

—  \ 

i  

-i  — 

—  -i 

(  — 

—2  — 

—  -i 

,  — 

—~\— 

—  \ 

f  

—  4— 

—  \ 

i  — 

—  5~ 

\ 

i  — 

-6- 

N 

s, 

S2 

S, 

S3 

S2 

s, 

S3 

S2 

s, 

S8 

S2 

s, 

S3 

S2 

s, 

S3 

s. 

s, 

S3 

S2 

S, 

4 

i 

3 

3 

I 

I 

4 

I 

i 

i 

4 

I 

3 

i 

I 

6 

i 

I 

2 

i 

2 

3 

4 

3 

i 

i 

i 

i 

3 

I 

I 

2 

3 

i 

i 

2 

I 

i 

X 

2 

2 

3 

I 

I 

3 

i 

I 

i 

i 

i 

ii 

i 

4 

8 

i 

I 

9 

2 

I 

7 

i 

3 

10 

2 

3 

II 

i 

2 

7 

4 

2 

2 

2 

i 

3 

I 

I 

2 

2 

2 

2 

2 

2 

I 

I 

I 

i 

2 

I 

3 

I 

I 

2 

i 

3 

i 

I 

I 

I 

2 

I 

•3 

3 

i 

I 

i 

2 

3 

2 

3 

2 

2 

3 

X 

I 

2 

3 

2 

3 

6 

I 

2 

2 

3 

I 

4 

18 

3 

8 

16 

2 

3 

16 

4 

6 

19 

6 

6 

16 

6 

6 

17 

5 

7 

18 

6 

6 

4 

2 

2 

I 

i 

2 

I 

i 

i 

i 

3 

I 

2 

i 

i 

2 

2 

i 

i 

4 

4 

i 

i 

2 

2 

i 

2 

2 

2 

I 

i 

5 

i 

2 

2 

I 

3 

2 

4 

2 

I 

4 

I 

4 

i 

2 

I 

4 

28 

4 

12 

24 

5 

5 

22 

7 

6 

29 

II 

8 

26 

6 

10 

23 

9 

IO 

23 

9 

10 

4 

i 

4 

i 

2 

2 

3 

i 

4 

i 

3 

i 

X 

3 

3 

2 

2 

2 

4 

3 

2 

I 

5 

3 

i 

3 

4 

i 

i 

5 

6 

3 

3 

2 

i 

3 

i 

4 

2 

2 

2 

2 

2 

2 

i 

3 

41 

8 

12 

35 

5 

8 

33 

II 

7 

41 

13 

10 

40 

8 

ii 

36 

II 

II 

34 

10 

u 

6 

I 

4 

i 

3 

i 

I 

i 

4 

4 

I 

I 

2 

X 

3 

2 

4 

i 

2 

i 

i 

4 

I 

2 

X 

i 

I 

5 

4 

i 

2 

4 

I 

2 

X 

7 

3 

3 

I 

2 

3 

I 

i 

4 

4 

I 

4 

i 

58 

8 

14 

44 

5 

9 

48 

12 

10 

Si 

15 

14 

51 

8 

ii 

52 

12 

15 

44 

ii 

17 

A  STUDY  OF  RETROACTIVE  INHIBITION  39 

The  figures  at  the  head  of  the  different  columns  correspond 
to  the  different  abscissae  of  the  curves.  Under  S3  appears  the 
number  of  wholly  correct  responses;  under  S2,  responses  with 
only  two  letters  correct;  and  under  S±,  responses  with  only  one 
letter  correct.  The  totals  at  the  end  of  each  cycle,  give  the  total 
number  of  responses,  under  the  different  columns,  up  to  that 
time,  and  not  the  totals  of  the  immediate  cycle  under  which 
they  are  placed.  The  writer's  opinion  is  that  more  is  to  be 
gained  from  a  consideration  of  this  table  than  from  a  considera- 
tion of  the  Curve  A. 

We  note  that  work  o,  when  compared  with  work  6,  has  the 
advantage  all  the  way  through,  save  in  the  second  cycle,  where 
the  reverse  is  the  case,  giving  at  the  end  of  the  second  cycle, 
where  n  =  56,  the  same  number  of  successful  responses  to  the 
credit  of  each.  A  point  of  great  significance  is  that  abscissa  o 
did  not  gain  its  ascendency  over  all  other  points  until  in  the 
fifth  cycle.  At  the  end  of  the  second  and  third  cycles,  abscissa  3 
was  in  the  ascendency  and  maintained  an  equality  at  the  end  of 
the  fourth  cycle,  losing  out  only  in  the  fifth  cycle.  Abscissa  6 
did  not  uniformly  occupy  the  lowest  position,  in  fact,  never  oc- 
cupying the  lowest  position  of  all.  At  the  end  of  the  fourth 
cycle,  n  =  112,  abscissae  6,  5,  2,  and  i  were  practically  on  an 
equal  footing.  These  considerations  seem  to  indicate  that  the 
ascendency  given  to  abscissa  o  at  the  end  of  the  fifth  cycle  is  not 
unquestionable.  In  fact,  the  results  of  the  fifth  cycle  seem  to  play 
an  unnatural  part,  as  respects  both  abscissae  o  and  6.  In  the 
light  of  the  results  of  the  first  four  cycles,  its  value  as  a  determi- 
nant of  the  final  figures,  seems  too  great.  Hence,  the  curve  from 
the  results  at  the  end  of  the  fifth  cycle  can  not  be  taken,  without 
question,  as  a  finality,  representing  the  true  status  of  the  matter. 

Again,  if  we  consider  the  part  of  Curve  A  on  each  side  of 
its  mid-abscissa,  i.e.  3,  we  get  the  total  number  of  successes  of 
the  half  towards  abscissa  o,  and  that  towards  abscissa  6,  equalling 
I75//2  and  172*^  respectively — their  difference  being  wholly  in- 
significant. This  is  an  important  fact,  highly  indicative,  that 
the  true  path  of  the  curve  is  along  the  horizontal,  and  conse- 
quently arguing  against  any  effect  of  retroactive  inhibition. 


40  /.  EDGAR  DECAMP 

For  a  consideration  of  further  details,  we  append  the  second 
table,  Table  X: 

TABLE  X--A  Series 
E.D.  D--    Di-      -i-        -it      - -t      D-t 

1  i 

2  I 

3  i    i 
4 

5 

6  i  i 

8  i  i 

9  i 
10        ii 

ii 

12  I 

13 

14  I 

15  2  I 

16 

17  I 

18  i        i 

19  i 

20  I  I 

21  21 

22  I 

23  2 

24 

25  2 

26  12 
27 

28  I  I 

29  I  I 

30  I 

31 

32  i 

33  i         i        2 

34  i 

35  2 

36  ill 

37  2 
38 

39  i    i 

40  i         i 

41  i 
42 

43  i 

44  i 

45  2 

46  I 
47 

48         i 

49 

50 

5i 

52 

53  i 


R. 

W. 

S3 

S2 

s, 

6 

o 

4 

I 

i 

5 

4 

I 

4 

2 

3 

i 

I 

3 

3 

4 

4 

2 

2 

5 

I 

i 

I 

6 

O 

6 

• 

o 

6 

i 

I 

i 

5 

2 

I 

2 

4 

3 

i 

I 

3 

3 

2 

2 

4 

3 

I 

5 

i 

2 

o 

6 

I 

i 

6 

o 

2 

2 

I 

5 

i 

3 

4 

2 

I 

I 

3 

3 

2 

2 

2 

4 

3 

I 

I 

5 

I 

I 

I 

O 

6 

2 

I 

2 

0 

6 

3 

I 

I 

5 

2 

2 

2 

4 

3 

3 

3 

3 

2 

4 

2 

2 

3 

5 

I 

3 

6 

O 

i 

2 

6 

O 

4 

2 

5 

I 

2 

I 

4 

2 

I 

3 

3 

2 

I 

2 

4 

I 

4 

I 

5 

4 

I 

0 

6 

I 

2 

o 

6 

2 

I 

I 

5 

i 

2 

2 

4 

5 

3 

3 

2 

I 

I 

6 

0 

3 

2 

4 

2 

2 

I 

5 

I 

4 

6 

0 

4 

I 

5 

I 

4 

I 

4 

2 

2 

2 

3 

3 

3 

I 

2 

4 

3 

I 

5 

2 

I 

O 

6 

5 

O 

6 

3 

I 

5 

6 

2 

4 

5 

3 

3 

i 

I 

A  STUDY  OF  RETROACTIVE  INHIBITION  41 

TABLE  X— A  Series  (continued) 
E.D.  D--    Di-      -i-        -it      - -t      D-t       R.        W.      S3        S2        S, 


17        29         4        18         4 

TABLE  X— B  Series 


4 

2 

3 

i 

5 

I 

i 

6 

O 

3             2 

6 

O 

6 

i 

5 

I 

4 

i 

4 

2 

3 

4 

2 

i           I 

i 

2 

4 

i 

I 

5 

4 

i 

O 

6 

2 

i 

0 

6 

2 

i 

I 

5 

4 

i 

2 

4 

2 

3 

3 

4 

i 

4 

2 

3          i 

2 

5 

I 

3 

6 

0 

7 

190        25 

53 

55% 

E.D. 

S3 

S2 

Si 

R. 

W. 

D-  -  Di- 

-i- 

i 

3 

i 

I 

0 

6 

i 

2 

3 

5 

i 

3 

I 

i 

I 

2 

4 

I 

4 

i 

2 

3 

3 

i    i 

I 

5 

3 

2 

4 

6 

4 

I 

5 

7 

3 

i 

O 

6 

i 

8 

i 

i 

I 

6 

0 

I 

9 

3 

5 

i 

10 

i 

i 

4 

2 

ii 

i 

I 

3 

3 

i 

12 

3 

4 

2 

13 

I 

i 

i 

5 

i 

14 

2 

6 

0 

2 

15 

2 

i 

I 

o 

6 

i 

I 

16 

2 

2 

i 

5 

I      2 

I 

17 

2 

2 

4 

I 

18 

I 

2 

I 

3 

3 

2 

I 

19 

3 

I 

4 

2 

I 

20 

2 

I 

5 

I 

I 

21 

I 

I 

6 

O 

I 

22 

3 

6 

O 

23 

I 

I 

5 

I 

I 

I 

24 

i 

2 

4 

2 

I 

25 

6 

I 

3 

3 

I 

26 

2 

2 

2 

4 

I     2 

I 

27 

3 

I 

I 

5 

I 

28 

4 

O 

6 

29 

I 

I 

O 

6 

I 

I 

30 

I 

I 

5 

I 

31 

2 

4 

32 

2 

2 

I 

3 

3 

I 

I 

33 

I 

4 

2 

34 

2 

I 

5 

I 

I 

-it      -  -t      D-t 

i 


42  /.   EDGAR  DECAMP 

TABLE  X— B  Series  (continued) 


E.D. 

S3 

S2 

& 

R. 

W. 

D--  Di-   -i-   -it 

35 

3 

I 

6 

o 

i 

36 

6 

0 

37 

2 

I 

5 

I 

i    i 

38 

2 

2 

4 

2 

2 

39 

4 

I 

3 

3 

I 

40 

4 

o 

6 

4i 

4 

2 

4 

42 

I 

2 

I 

I 

5 

2     I 

43 

I 

I 

O 

6 

I 

44 

3 

I 

5 

45 

4 

I 

2 

4 

I 

46 

4 

3 

3 

47 

2 

2 

4 

2 

I           I 

48 

3 

2 

5 

I 

I           I 

49 

3 

6 

O 

50 

3 

I 

6 

O 

I 

51 

3 

5 

I 

52 

4 

4 

2 

53 

4 

2 

3 

3 

I           I 

54 

2 

2 

2 

4 

I           I 

55 

2 

2 

I 

5 

2 

56 

2 

I 

3 

O 

6 

I           21 

57 

2 

I 

O 

6 

I 

58 

4 

I 

I 

I 

5 

I      I 

59 

4 

2 

4 

60 

2 

I 

3 

3 

I 

61 

4 

I 

4 

2 

I 

62 

2 

5 

I 

63 

3 

6 

O 

64 

i 

I 

6 

O 

I 

65 

5 

I 

66 

5 

4 

2 

67 

3 

I 

I 

3 

3 

I      I 

68 

4 

2 

4 

69 

4 

I 

0 

6 

I 

70 

4 

I 

I 

o 

I 

158 

46 

37 

9   32   28    12 

-  -t      D-t 


45% 

The  meaning  of  the  symbols  used  has  been  given  under 
Experiment  II.  A  few  partial  displacements  appear  in  the  cyclic 
order,  but,  due  to  the  brevity  of  the  cycle,  this  appears 
negligible.  Under  E.  D.  60,  it  will  be  noted  that  R.  4,  W.  2  has 
been  reckoned  as  R.  3,  W.  3.  This  appears  permissible,  since,  had 
the  additional  minute  of  work  been  engaged  in,  the  result  could 
hardly  have  fallen  lower  in  that  particular  instant.  Even  as  it 
was,  the  result  is  below  that  of  the  B  series  with  the  same 
point  on  the  same  day. 

2S3  of  the  A  series  equals  190,  while  2S3  of  the  B  series 
equals  only  162,  the  percentages  being  54.6  and  45.4  respec- 


A  STUDY  OF  RETROACTIVE  INHIBITION  43 

tively.  In  explanation  of  this  difference,  we  may  refer  to  our 
discussion  under  Experiment  I.  We  note  here,  as  previously, 
that  the  Di-  type  of  partial  successes  far  outstrips  the  -it  type, 
the  former  giving,  in  toto,  49;  and  the  latter,  16.  The  D-t 
type  is  fairly  negligible,  giving,  in  all,  only  six  occurrences. 
In  the  one-letter-correct  syllables,  the  vowels  far  exceed  either 
the  initial  or  the  final  consonant;  in  fact,  the  vowel  occurs 
oftener  than  both  combined. 

If  we  take  the  total  number  of  successes,  i.e.,  2S3(A)  plus 
2S3(B)  =  348,  and  consider  them  relatively  to  the  position  they 
occupied  in  the  learning,  we  get  the  following  distribution 
according  to  measures : 

Position  Responses 

1  58 

2  40 

3  48 

4  45 

5  49 

6  44 

7  64 

These  results  seem  to  indicate,  as  those  in  Experiment  II,  that 
syllables  occupying  the  first  and  seventh  measures,  are  more 
firmly  associated  in  the  learning  process.  Further,  the  seventh 
position  seems  to  possess  this  advantage  to  the  greatest  extent. 
The  results  of  Experiment  I  do  not  accord  upon  this  latter  point. 
In  explanation,  it  may  be  suggested  that  the  beginning  and  end 
syllables  have  special  attention  called  to  them,  in  that  they  begin 
and  end  the  series.  But  since  the  repetitions  are  continuous, 
and  there  is  no  break  at  the  end  of  the  seventh  position  different 
from  that  at  the  end  of  any  other  position,  except,  of  course, 
at  the  closing  of  the  learning  process,  this  attentive  distinction 
seems  a  rather  meagre  explanation  for  the  disparity  at  hand. 
A  second  explanation  offers  itself.  Regardless  of  the  contin- 
uity of  the  learning  process,  the  subject  is  nevertheless  aware, 
as  the  repetitions  continue,  of  the  end  and  beginning  of  the 
series.  Hence,  a  characteristic  difference  qualifying  positions 
7  and  i.  Without  mentioning  the  results,  the  subject 
of  the  present  experiment  was  questioned  about  the  consciousness 
of  such  a  distinction.  The  reply  was  in  the  negative.  A 


44  /.  EDGAR  DECAMP 

knowledge  of  the  position  in  the  series  occupied  by  a  syllable, 
with  Z.  B.,  seemed  to  be  much  less  than  that  possessed  by 
subject  F.,  in  Experiment  II.  The  second  explanation  seems 
valid  in  case  of  the  results  with  subject  F.,  but  appears  some- 
what lacking  in  explanation  of  the  greater  difference  obtained 
with  Z.  B.,  unless  we  assume  that  the  same  attentive  distinction 
was  present,  but  subliminal.  Even  at  that,  we  should  hardly 
expect  the  difference  to  be  greater  in  the  latter  case.  Subject 
B.  of  Experiment  I  does  not  show  the  characteristic  under  dis- 
cussion with  respect  to  the  first  position.  He  does  show  it 
in  case  of  the  seventh  position.  No  reason  for  the  difference 
in  case  of  subject  B.  is  forthcoming,  unless  we  ascribe  it  to  the 
insufficiency  of  numbers. 

The  learning  process  (Vorzeigen),  used  by  Muller  and 
Pilzecker,1  for  the  most  part,  gave  between  the  last  and  the 
first  syllable  of  a  series,  a  longer  interval  than  occurred  between 
any  other  two  adjacent  syllables.  Such  a  procedure,  we  may 
assume,  slightly  accentuates  the  importance  of  the  last  and  first 
syllables.  Their  Experiment  28  was  conducted  with  no  such  dis- 
tinguishing difference  qualifying  the  last  and  first  syllables.  The 
order  of  the  positions  occupied  by  the  syllables  in  the  learning, 
in  accordance  with  the  largeness  of  the  number  of  syllables 
obtained,  was  5,  4,  3,  6,  2,  i. 

If  we  turn  to  a  consideration  of  the  time  curve,  we  find,  as 
in  Experiments  I  and  II,  no  apparent  general  relation  between 
the  time  and  the  success  curves.  In  four  instances  there  is 
concordance  of  direction,  in  two  instances  there  is  opposition. 
If  we  except  the  fifth  abscissa,  the  general  trend  of  the  curve 
is  downward.  It  is  to  be  noted,  that  with  the  two  highest  points 
of  the  success  curve  correspond  the  two  highest  points  of  the 
time  curve,  and  with  the  lowest  point  of  the  success  curve 
corresponds  the  lowest  point  of  the  time  curve.  These  results 
militate  against  any  general  statement  of  the  relation  of  the 
success  and  time  curves  as  one  of  inverse  variance.  Our  average 
time  is  somewhat  longer  than  that  obtained  by  Muller  and 

1  Loc.  dt.,  S.  266. 


A  STUDY  OF  RETROACTIVE  INHIBITION  45 

Pilzecker.  This  appears  partially  explicable  in  that  in  the  present 
experiment  the  average  percentage  of  successful  syllables  was 
fairly  low.  The  number  of  repetitions  was  restricted  lest  the 
subject  should  too  often  get  all  the  response  syllables  correct. 
This  would  have  a  tendency  to  give  comparatively  long  times. 
That  the  explanation  is  adequate  for  the  full  difference,  can 
scarcely  be  affirmed  or  denied. 

Constructing  a  table  showing  the  average  time  under  each 
abscissa  at  the  end  of  each  cycle  we  have  Table  XL 

TABLE  XI 
Work         0123456 

Cycle  I 7172  3393  3782  3816  5431  8460  3867 

6123574 
Cycle  2 5216  4108  5998  4945  6594  6518  4629 

4153762 
Cycle  3 5866  5502  5400  5633  6328  6518  4402 

5324671 
Cycle  4.  •• -6451  6544  6381  5786  5570  6557  474<> 

5  6  4  3  71 

Cycle  5.... 7039  6756  5625  6548  5967  7773  5706 

6514372 

The  italicized  figures  represent  the  order  of  the  length  of 
time  of  the  particular  abscissa  in  the  particular  group.  It  is 
noticeable  that  abscissa  five  maintained  the  highest  position 
throughout  save  at  the  end  of  the  second  cycle,  when  it  occupied 
the  sixth.  Abscissa  6  twice  occupied  the  first  position,  and 
never  above  the  fourth.  Abscissa  o  twice  occupied  the  sixth  posi- 
tion, and  never  below  the  fourth.  Abscissa  3  seemed  to  hold 
a  median  position  throughout. 

Eleven  per  cent  of  the  total  successes  possess  the  characteristic 
of  having  been  given  erroneously  as  responses  to  test  syllables, 
previously  to  their  being  given  in  the  correct  position.  These 
responses,  when  appearing  in  the  correct  position,  usually 
elicited  long  times,  the  average  time  being  109340-.  It  is  very 
probable  that  this  long  average  time  arose  from  the  inhibitive 
influence  occasioned  by  having  given  the  syllable  in  response 
to  a  previous  test  syllable. 

It  might  be  suggested  that  the  long  average  time  was  due 
to  some  few  very  long  unnatural  times,  due  to  chance  occur- 
rences. In  order  to  throw  light  upon  this  point  a  table  is 


46  /.  EDGAR  DECAMP 

presented  showing  the  distribution  of  the  responses  according 
to  their  time   length. 


TABLE   XII 


Work 

o 

i 

2 

3 

4 

5 

6 

T  <  looo      

i 

i 

i 

1000  <  T  <   2000  

13 

9 

12 

6 

16 

9 

8 

2000  <  T  <   3000  

8 

9 

6 

ii 

4 

12 

ii 

3000  <  T  <  4000  

ii 

3 

9 

5 

5 

4 

7 

4000  <  T  <  5000  

i 

3 

5 

3 

6 

4 

3 

5000  <  T  <  6000  

3 

i 

3 

5 

3 

i 

6000  <  T  <  7000  

2 

2 

3 

2 

3 

2 

7000  <  T  <  8000  

3 

4 

2 

i 

2 

8000  <  T  <  oooo  

i 

2 

i 

I 

2 

2 

9000  <  T  <  loooo  

I 

i 

2 

i 

2 

3 

loooo  <  T  <  i  looo  

I 

I 

I  IOOO  <  T  <  I200O  

I 

2 

I 

I 

12000  <  T  <  13000  

I 

2 

13000  <  T  <  14000  

I 

I 

I 

3 

I 

14000  <  T  <  15000  

2 

I 

I 

3 

I 

i 

15000  <  T  <  20000  

I 

2 

2 

2 

3 

3 

I 

20000  <  T  <  30000  

5 

3 

I 

2 

2 

4 

I 

30000  <  T  <  40000  

i 

2 

I 

The  arrangement  is  self-explanatory.  The  number  of  long 
times  is  rather  large,  except  in  case  of  abscissae  6  and  2,  to 
be  disregarded  on  the  basis  of  unnaturalness.  Abscissae  6 
and  2,  if  considered  in  isolation,  may  appear  to  give  weight 
to  this  idea.  To  be  sure,  for  adequate  comparison  of  the  num- 
bers under  different  abscissae,  the  total  number  for  each  point 
should  be  the  same.  However,  the  difference  is  slight,  and  if 
we  correct  the  table  to  a  standard  of  50  as  a  total,  proportion- 
ately to  the  present  distribution,  the  correcting  figure  would  in 
no  case  exceed  2,  usually,  not  approaching  2  save  in  one 
or  two  cases  under  each  abscissa.  Bearing  this  correction  in 
mind,  we  note  that  for  T  <  20000  the  order  of  the  abscissae, 
according  to  brevity  of  time,  is  4,  o,  2,  i,  5,  6,  3.  For  T  <  30000-, 
this  order  is  i,  6,  5,  o,  {43.  T  <  40000-,  o,  6,  2,  i,  5, 
4,  3.  T  <  50000-,  2,  6,  o,  4/5,  i,  3. 

Plotting  a  curve  for  all  those  times  less  than  40000-  and  another 
curve  for  all  those  times  less  than  30000-  we  obtain  the  following, 
Figs.  6  and  7: 

The  curves  fluctuate  about  the  horizontal.  This  fluctuation 
is  exceedingly  slight.  The  maximum  difference  obtainable  be- 
tween two  points  in  the  curve  of  Figs.  6  is  283^,  in  the  curve 


A  STUDY  OF  RETROACTIVE  INHIBITION 


47 


JTooo 


if.  ooo 


JOOO 


2,000 


/ooo 


,     3 
FIG.  6 


#000 


JOOO 


Zooa 


FIG.  7 

of  Fig.  7,  4060-.  According  to  the  general  conception  of  retro- 
active inhibition,  the  present  time  curves,  as  well  as  the  gen- 
eral time  curve,  should  gradually  rise  as  the  abscissae  increase. 
We  find  no  such  tendency  unquestionably  manifesting  itself. 
The  curve  of  Fig.  6  ends  at  abscissa  6,  1340-  higher  than  its 
starting  point.  The  curve  of  Fig.  7  ends  200-  lower  than  its 
starting  point. 

Z.  B.  was  experienced  in  learning  nonsense  syllables.     It  can 


48  /.   EDGAR  DECAMP 

not  be  said  that  her  learning  of  them  was  free  from  sense 
associations,  yet  she  was  not  given  to  consistently  forming  them 
as  an  aid  in  remembering  the  syllables.  Occasionally  it  was 
noticed  that  an  associated  (sense)  syllable  gave  quite  a  long 
reaction  time.  In  such  cases  the  mental  process  seemed  to  be. 
something  like  this:  The  subject  recalled  that  in  the  learning 
there  was  a  sense  association  connected  with  the  test  syllable 
but,  somehow,  it  had  dropped  out,  and  time  was  consumed  in 
search  'for  the  sense  association.  Sometimes  the  mere  fact 
of  an  established  sense  association  would  lead  to  an  incorrect 
response.  The  association  would  work  but,  due  to  its  sense 
relation,  might  give  some  word  that  would  fit  the  association 
just  as  well.  In  a  few  instances  it  was  noted  that  sense  associa- 
tions, established  during  learning,  did  not  consciously  work  for 
the  recall,  even  though  the  correct  response  was  given.  Whether 
the  sense  association  worked  as  a  subliminal  mediate  association, 
or  whether  the  pure  association  was  strong  enough  to  recall 
the  correct  response,  cannot  be  definitely  stated. 

The  subject  thought  she  noticed  variation  in  the  different 
series  of  syllables  with  respect  to  the  difficulty  of  learning 
them,  characterizing  some  as  hard  and  some  as  comparatively 
easy. 

The  subject  was  repeatedly  questioned  relative  to  any  notice- 
able influence  of  the  interpolated  work  upon  the  learned 
syllables.  Characteristic  responses  are,  "Noticed  no  influence 
of  the  work,"  "Didn't  notice  influence  of  work."  Sometimes 
the  subject  would  be  surprised  at  the  result  at  the  end  of  the 
test,  having  expected  to  give  a  greater  (or  less)  number  of 
correct  responses. 

In  cases  when  most,  or  a  very  few,  of  the  responses  were 
correct,  the  subject  was  questioned  for  any  apparent  reason 
for  such  a  result.  None  was  apparent.  In  the  only  case  where 
all  the  responses  were  correctly  given,  W.  o,  R.  15,  the  subject 
characterized  the  result  as  a  "Happy  accident." 

There  were  comparatively  few  recurrences  of  measures  in  the 
variant  interval,  an  average  of  about  one  for  every  two  series 
learned.  The  majority  of  these  occurred  during  the  variant 


A  STUDY  OF  RETROACTIVE  INHIBITION  49 

interval  R.  15,  or  in  the  rest  period  previous  to  any  work  engaged 
in.  As  many  recurred  with  R.  15  as  recurred  in  the  first  rest 
interval  of  all  the  other  five  positions,  where  work  followed 
a  short  rest  period.  About  one-third  oi  the  recurrences  came 
in  the  rest  period  following  work.  Only  two  measures  and 
one  isolated  syllable  are  reported  as  having  recurred  during  the 
work  period.  The  measures  reported  as  recurring  almost  in- 
variably gave  rise  to  correct  responses  in  the  test.  Three  in- 
stances are  recorded  to  the  contrary.  These  three  recurred 
in  the  rest  period  following  work.  It  may  be  said  that  there 
is  slight  evidence  in  favor  of  the  view  that  the  advantage  shown 
by  the  R.  type  over  the  W.  6,  R.  9  type  was  not  due  to  the 
retroactive  effect,  in  the  latter  case,  but  due  to  cutting  off  a  few 
additional  repetitions  of  some  of  the  measures  by  the  work 
introduced,  while  in  the  former  case,  a  few  additional  repeti- 
tions went  on  unhindered.  A  consciousness  of  any  such  tendency 
towards  a  continuance  of  the  repetition  of  the  syllables,  im- 
mediately after  the  learning  process,  was  denied  by  the  subject. 
In  fact,  the  subject's  opinion  was  that  the  syllables  became  a 
kind  of  nonentity  during  the  rest  period,  the  syllables,  per 
se,  not  having  any  thing  particularly  attractive  about  them, 
tended  to  drop  out  of  mind,  while  necessarily  the  mind  wandered 
into  easy  channels  of  thought. 

The  number  of  successful  responses  for  the  first  and  seventh 
positions  was,  for  R.  o,  W.  6,  9  and  6  respectively;  for  R.  6, 
W.  o,  5  and  13.  This  appears  slightly  in  favor  of  the  view 
just  referred  to.  When  the  remaining  experiments,  subsequent 
as  well  as  foregoing,  are  considered  in  this  respect,  the  seventh 
position  usually  has  a  tendency  to  give  a  few  more  results  with 
rest  than  with  work  immediately  following  the  learning.  This 
tendency,  however,  is  not  sufficiently  great  to  point  to  the  inter- 
ference by  the  work  with  additional  subliminal  repetitions  as 
being  more  than  a  secondary  factor,  or  otherwise  as  one  of 
several  primary  factors,  in  explaining  the  difference  at  hand. 


50  /.  EDGAR  DECAMP 

EXPERIMENT  IX  (b) 

This  experiment  is  a  combination  of  Experiment  IX  (a),  with 
the  same  subject.  Two  variations,  however,  are  introduced; 
one  with  respect  to  the  variant  interval,  the  other  with  respect 
to  the  kind  of  work  used  in  the  variant  interval.  The  subject 
either  rested  the  whole  of  the  variant  interval,  or  she  worked 
six  minutes  and  rested  nine  minutes  before  testing.  The  work, 
in  the  latter  case,  came  immediately  after  the  learning  of  a 
series.  The  comparison  series  was  learned  and  tested  after 
fifteen  minutes  rest.  On  one  day  the  series  with  work  follow- 
ing was  learned  and  tested  first,  on  the  next  day  the  comparison 
series  was  given  the  first  place.  Cross-multiplication  was  no 
longer  used  as  mental  activity  for  producing  the  retroactive 
effect  upon  a  learned  series.  For  the  work,  or  mental  activity, 
the  subject  was  given  a  problem  to  solve.  Such  problems  were 
used  as  one  would  class  as  of  the  recreation  type.  A  new  problem 
was  given  each  day. 

The  experiment  continued  eight  experiment  days,  with  the 
following  results,  Table  XIII,  n  =  56 : 

TABLE  XIII 

S3      r      m.v.      %  S2  $!  Tr   T<2OOO(r  T<isoo<r 

Cs.  I.  Rest  15  30    3.67     1.5      53.6  3    3  5238          n  5 

Os.  II.    Work  6,  Rest  9.. 26    3.25      .94    46.4  2    4  5593  4  o 

We  again  note  a  slight  advantage  in  favor  of  R.  15.  With 
the  greater  number  of  successful  responses  is  correlated  a 
shorter  average  reaction  time.  With  the  exception  of  the  greater 
m.  v.  for  Case  I,  the  results  point  towards  a  slightly  injurious 
effect  of  the  mental  activity  following  the  learning  of  a  series. 

If  we  consider  the  first  four  experiment  days,  the  number  of 
successes  for  Case  I  and  for  Case  II  are  17  and  13  respectively. 
For  the  last  four  experiment  days  the  corresponding  figures 
are  13  and  13,  thereby  indicating  that  the  whole  disadvantage 
of  Case  II  appeared  in  the  first  group  of  four  experiment  days, 
and  was  not  equally  distributed  throughout  the  whole  eight  days. 

Table  XIV  is  presented  for  further  considerations. 


A  STUDY  OF  RETROACTIVE  INHIBITION  51 

TABLE  XIV 

A  Series 

D  W        R        D--    Di-     -i-       -it      --t      D-t       S3        S2        Sx 

1691  41 

2  O  15  I  51 
369                                          2  12 

4  o        15  6 

569  I  41 

6  O  15  I  21 

769  3 

o        15  i  6         i 

241  3i          3         4 

B   Series 

D                   W        R        D--    Di-     -i-  -it      --t      D-t       S3         Ss        S, 

i                   o        15  3 

2691  41 

3  o        15                              2  32 
469  4 

5  o        15  i 

669  I  21 

7  o        15  i  4i 
8691  41 

123  25          2          4 

Comment  upon  this  table  is  scarcely  necessary,  since  it  con- 
forms with  the  results  shown  in  Table  X.  Consequently,  the 
discussion  presented  in  connection  with  Table  X  is  applicable 
to  the  present  table. 

If  we  now  combine  the  present  results  with  those  of  Experi- 
ment IX  (a),  we  greatly  increase  the  comparative  figures  for 
the  two  cases,  viz.,  (i)  learning,  rest  fifteen  minutes,  test  and 
(2)  learning,  work  six  minutes,  rest  nine  minutes,  test.  We 
thereby  obtain  comparative  results  for  the  two  cases  when 
n  =  196.  The  different  magnitudes  appear  in  Table  XV. 

TABLE  XV 

S,  %  Tr 

Cs.  I.    Rest  15 88  55.7  6382 

Cs.  II.    Work  6,  Rest  9 70  44.3  5667 

In  five  of  the  seven  cycles,  four  results  each,  of  the  com- 
bined results,  the  number  of  .successes  for  R.  15  slightly  exceeds 
that  for  R.  o,  W.  6,  R.  9. 

From  the  introspective  side  little  can  be  added.  The  subject 
reported  that  she  did  not  notice  any  inhibitive  effect  of  the 
mental  activity  engaged  in  after  learning  a  series.  She  was 
of  the  opinion  that  the  new  kind  of  material  used  for  mental 


52  /.  EDGAR  DECAMP 

activity  did  not  noticeably  command  her  attention  more  than 

the  cross-multiplication. 

i 

EXPERIMENT  X 

The  general  plan  of  this  experiment  was  the  same  as  that 
of  Experiment  IX ;  ten  repetitions  were  used.  There  intervened 
between  the  learning  and  the  test  of  each  series  of  syllables 
fifteen  minutes,  the  last  nine  of  which  were  always  spent  as 
a  rest  period.  In  the  first  six  minutes  cross-multiplication,  as 
interpolated  work,  was  introduced,  the  amount  varying  with  the 
cyclic  order  as  already  outlined.  Two  series  were  learned  on 
each  experiment  day. 

After  one  and  one-half  cycles  the  subject,  A.,  could  spare  no 
more  time  for  the  work.  The  experiment  closed  with  n  =  42,  or 
six  tests  for  each  point  of  the  six  minutes  varied.  The  results 
are  presented  below  in  Table  XVI : 

TABLE  XVI 

S3  Tr 

15  1249 

17  2196 
19  1762 

18  2451 
23  1762 
12  1689 
14  2500 

The  first  column  contains  the  disposition  of  the  variant  in- 
terval. Corresponding  to  this,  the  second  column  contains  the 
total  number  of  successes,  and  the  third  column  contains  the 
average  time  for  the  correct  responses. 

The  experiment  was  not  carried  to  sufficient  length  to  permit 
of  great  stress  upon  the  results  obtained.  However,  it  seems 
apparent  that  there  is  no  trace  of  a  tendency  towards  the  appear- 
ance of  retroactive  inhibition.  In  fact,  we  should  infer  that  a 
modicum  of  work  aids  in  producing  a  large  number  of  successes. 
A  peculiar  departure  exists  in  that  with  the  most  (least)  work 
the  average  time  appears  the  shortest  (longest). 

It  should  be  mentioned  that  A.  could  scarcely  avoid  sense 
associations  to  connect  the  two  syllables  of  a  measure.  Indeed, 
it  was  the  exception,  rather  than  the  general  rule,  for  him  to 


J 

e 

O.  . 

2. 

4, 

9.  . 

«. 

7, 

9.  . 

4, 

2, 

9.  . 

5> 

I, 

9.  . 

6. 

0. 

O.. 

A  STUDY  OF  RETROACTIVE  INHIBITION  53 

get  a  correct  response  that  had  no  sense  connection  with  its 
paired  syllable. 

EXPERIMENT  XI 

P.  served  as  subject  for  the  present  experiment.  The  general 
plan  is  the  same  as  that  of  Experiment  III;  eight  repetitions 
were  used.  The  length  of  the  variant  interval  was  fifteen 
minutes.  A  comparison  was  planned  between  only  two  condi- 
tions, between  results  obtained  when  no  work  was  used  in  the 
variant  interval,  and  those  obtained  when  the  variant  interval 
was  thus  disposed :  Immediately  after  learning,  ten  minutes  work, 
followed  by  five  minutes  rest.  The  work  used  was  playing  chess. 
The  operator  had  the  chess-men  in  position  previous  to  the 
learning  of  a  set  of  syllables,  that  were  to  be  followed  by  work 
so  that  the  play  might  begin  without  delay.  Two  sets  of 
syllables  were  learned  and  tested  on  each  experiment  day.  At 
least  five  minutes  intervened  between  the  close  of  the  test  of 
the  first  set  and  the  beginning  of  the  learning  of  the  second  set. 
On  one  day  the  experiment  began  with  the  work  type,  on  the 
next  day  with  the  rest  type.  The  experiment  continued  ten 
experiment  days  with  the  following  results.  Table  XVII,  n  =  70 : 

TABLE  XVII 

S3       r       m.v.  %  S2  S,  Tr  T<  15000- 

Cs.  I.     Rest  15 28      2.8      1.44  40  9  "  2343            14 

Cs.  II.     Chess  10;  Rest  5 35      3.5      i.io  50  9  6  2392            10 

In  the  light  of  the  explanation  of  the  previous  tables,  the 
symbols  used  above  require  no  further  explanation. 

Here  extraordinary  results  appear  overbalancing  the  scale  in 
favor  of  Case  II.  The  m.  v.  of  Case  II  is  slightly  less  than  that 
of  Case  I.  The  average  time  of  the  successful  responses  is  490- 
longer  for  Case  II  than  for  Case  I.  Case  II  is  slightly  favored 
in  the  number  of  successes  giving  time  less  than  15000-. 

In  the  present  experiment  the  time  'factor  gives — and  then 
very  meagrely — the  only  possible  indication  of  the  deleterious 
influence  of  retroactive  inhibition.  The  number  of  successes — 
unquestionably  the  deciding  criterion — instead  of  lending  weight 
to  such  an  influence  of  retroactive  inhibition,  distinctly  nullifies 


54  /.   EDGAR  DECAMP 

its  possibility.  In  the  opinion  of  the  writer,  the  advantage  shown 
by  Case  II  is  not  to  be  interpreted  as  showing  that  the  pro- 
cedure of  Case  II  is  the  more  productive  of  successes  in  general. 
It  seems  that  this  advantage  finds  sufficient  explanation  in  terms 
of  chance  variation  in  the  syllables,  experimental  method,  etc. 
Relatively  to  the  work  used,  it  may  be  remarked  that  there 
occurs  to  the  writer  no  mental  employment  that  holds  the  atten- 
tion more  slavishly  than  does  the  game  of  chess. 


It  will  be  remembered  that  in  all  of  our  previous  experiments 
only  experienced  subjects  were  used.  From  a  consideration  of 
the  possibility  that  subjects,  inexperienced  in  learning  nonsense 
syllables,  might  show  quite  different  results  from  those  shown 
by  experienced  subjects,  the  present  experiment  consisted  in 
carrying  out  the  previously  adopted  general  plan  with  34 — n 
women,  23  men — laboratory  students,  inexperienced  in  learning 
nonsense  syllables.  Each  subject  served  two  experiment  days. 
Two  tests  were  given  him  on  each  experiment  day.  Seven- 
measure  syllables,  two  .syllables  to  the  measure,  were  given 
twelve  repetitions  for  each  subject.  The  variant  interval — 
fifteen  minutes — found  distribution  into  two  types,  a  rest  (R.) 
type,  consisting  in  rest  throughout  the  variant  interval;  and  a 
work  (W.)  type,  consisting  in  working  six  minutes  immediately 
following  the  learning  of  a  set  of  syllables,  then  resting  the 
remaining  nine  minutes  of  the  variant  interval.  The  work  used 
was  three  problems  of  the  recreation  type. 

The  subjects  were  informed  concerning  the  experiment  only 
sufficiently  to  enable  them  to  carry  out  their  part  of  the  ex- 
periment. At  no  time  was  it  hinted  that  the  operator's  problem 
dealt  with  retroactive  inhibition,  or  the  comparison  of  results 
following  work  and  rest.  Several  had  the  idea  that  some  test 
of  their  memory  was  being  made.  The  subject  did  not  know, 
until  the  end  of  the  learning  process,  whether  he  was  to  work 
or  rest.  The  operator  informed  him  at  the  beginning  that  he 
(the  operator)  would  give  him  a  signal,  either  "Work"  or 
"Rest."  In  the  case  of  "Rest"  being  given,  he  was  to  abstain 


A  STUDY  OF  RETROACTIVE  INHIBITION  55 

from  any  particular  mental  thought;  in  case  of  "Work,"  he 
should  turn  over  the  previously  prepared  sheet  of  problems, 
select  one  of  the  three  and  work  hard  until  the  signal  for  stopping 
was  given.  The  sheet  of  paper,  containing  the  three  problems 
written  on  one  side  and  the  title  "Problems"  written  on  the 
other,  was  placed,  with  the  written  problems  down,  in  easy 
reach  of  the  subject.  A  sheet  containing  three  problems  dif- 
ferent from  those  used  the  first  experiment  day  was  used  for 
the  second  experiment  day. 

The  two  experiment  days  of  each  subject  were  separated  by 
no  constant  time  interval.  It  was  considered  necessary  for 
each  subject  to  maintain  the  same  hour  of  the  day  in  his  two 
experiment  days.  This  was  departed  from  in  only  three  in- 
stances, and  then  the  displacement  was  only  one  hour. 

In  order  to  maintain  an  equal  footing  for  each  of  the  two 
types  (R.  and  W.),  seventeen  of  the  subjects  began  their  first 
experiment  day  with  the  R.  type;  and  seventeen  with  the  W. 
type.  Those  beginning  the  first  day  with  the  R.  (W.)  type 
began  the  second  day  with  the  W  (R.)  type. 

The  results  obtained  are  abridged  in  Tables  XVIII  (a), 
XVIII  (b),  and  XVIII  (c)  following: 


TABLE  XVIII   (a) 


D. 

No.     i W 

2 R 

3 R 

4 R 

5 W 

6 W 

7 R 

8 R 

9 R 

10 R 

ii R 

T.  G.  A. 


Type  R  ^    i  

—Type  W  > 

s, 

S2 

S3 

Tr 

s, 

S2 

S3 

Tr 

5 

7 

1848 

I 

2 

7 

3175 

i 

10 

2432 

6 

2317 

i 

5 

2739 

I 

6 

7739 

i 

3 

6 

1180 

2 

5 

1099 

i 

9 

1285 

4 

2 

6 

1830 

i 

6 

4939 

I 

7 

2173 

i 

i 

5 

2818 

i 

I 

6 

1380 

2 

12 

1856 

2 

10 

1747 

2 

2 

5 

1059 

5 

4308 

I 

9 

1397 

i 

8 

1317 

3 

2 

5 

1873 

I 

I 

10 

1806 

9 

18 

79 

2120 

12 

8 

76 

2232 

/.  EDGAR  DECAMP 


No.  i.. 

2.. 
3" 

4-- 

5-. 
6.. 

7-. 

8.. 

9-. 
10.. 
ii.. 

12.  . 

13.. 
14.. 

IS-- 
16.. 
17-. 
18.. 
19.. 
20.. 

21.  . 

22.  . 
23-. 

T.  G.  A. 


TABLE 

XVIII  (b) 

/  Type  R  » 

D. 

S, 

S2 

s, 

Tr 

R 

i 

2 

8 

1231 

W 

i 

6 

0830 

R 

i 

2 

3 

O906 

W 

10 

2993 

W 

2 

5 

2329 

W 

I 

6 

2670 

W 

2 

I 

5 

0742 

W 

I 

10 

1680 

W 

2 

3 

6 

1636 

R 

3 

i 

2 

? 

W 

2 

3 

? 

R 

2 

i 

3 

Il87 

W 

i 

3 

1922 

R 

I 

i 

6 

262O 

W 

2 

4 

0787 

R 

i 

5 

1377 

R 

3 

i 

3948 

R 

i 

i 

8 

2689 

W 

i 

3 

6 

2546 

R 

i 

i 

4 

I7IO 

W 

i 

2 

5 

1332 

W 

i 

I 

6 

1587 

W 

I 

ii 

3062 

26 

28 

123 

2OO2 

/ Type  W 

St   S2  S3    T 
2   i   7 


2 

I 
22 


6 

I 

IO 

5 
4 
7 
7 
6 

2 
2 
2 

6 

4 
5 
4 

10 
ii 

3 

10 

6 

6 


29  124 


1086 
0916 

? 

1348 
2707 
1968 
1326 
1502 
1849 
4922 
1737 

IIO2 
I047I 

1057 
0862 

1644 
2156 
4697 
1433 
1764 
1700 
1969 


Comb.  T.  G.  A.. 


TABLE  XVIII   (c) 
, Type  R.- 

Oi      02      Oj      1  r 

35   46   202   2047 


s, 

32 


-Type  W.- 

S2    S3 
37   200 


Table  XVIII  (a)  shows  the  results  for  the  eleven  women. 
Table  XVIII  (b)  gives  the  results  for  the  twenty-three  men.  In 
the  column  D.  is  to  be  found  the  type — whether  R.  (rest)  or  W. 
(work) — beginning  the  first  experiment  day.  T.  G.  A.  =  Totals 
and  General  Average,  the  latter  referring  to  the  time  only.  In 
Table  XVIII  (c)  the  results  of  Tables  XVIII  (a)  and  XVIII  (b) 
are  briefly  combined.  Under  Tr  is  found  the  average  time, 
obtained  by  totalling  the  times  for  the  successful  responses  for 
the  R.  (or  W.)  type  and  dividing  by  the  number  of  successful 
responses. 

It  is  obvious  that  the  results  of  any  particular  subject,  con- 
sidered alone,  are  of  little  value.  However,  when  the  results 
of  the  different  experiments  are  considered  in  relation  to  each 
other,  or  in  toto,  it  may  be  possible  to  approach  some  general 
conclusion.  We  reverse  the  order  and  deal  with  the  latter  first. 

First.     The  total  number  of  successes  for  the  R.  type  was 


A  STUDY  OF  RETROACTIVE  INHIBITION  57 

202,  that  for  the  W.  type  was  200.  There  exists  a  difference, 
wholly  insignificant.  The  average  time  shows  a  slight  increase — 
i37<r — in  type  W.  over  that  of  type  R.  If  we  glance  at  the 
time  results  of  XVIII  (b)  we  note  that  No.  14  apparently  gives 
a  "freak"  result,  with  respect  to  the  W.  type,  in  that  it  is  so 
far  different  from  any  other  average  obtained.  If  we  neglect 
the  results  of  No.  14,  our  average,  in  toto,  for  the  R.  type  is 
26290-,  and  for  the  W.  type,  19830-.  This  modified  form  appears 
more  nearly  correct,  otherwise  one  result,  decidedly  outside  the 
limit  of  variation,  would  exert  too  great  a  determining  influence 
upon  our  comparative  averages.  It  appears  then  that  we  may 
say,  from  the  standpoint  of  the  results  considered  in  toto,  neither 
the  number  of  successes  nor  the  average  time  is  indicative  of 
any  deleterious  influence  of  retroactive  inhibition. 

Secondly.  If  we  consider  the  different  results  in  relation  to 
each  other,  we  find  the  following.  Of  the  thirty-four  subjects 
there  are  fourteen,  each  of  whom  gives  more  successes  for  the 
R.  type  than  for  the  W.  type.  There  are  twelve,  each  of  whom 
gives  more  successes  for  the  W.  type  than  for  the  R.  type. 
Eight  of  the  thirty-four  are  neutral.  This  seems  adequate  proof 
that  there  is  no  general  tendency  towards  favoring  either  type. 
Further,  with  respect  to  time,  sixteen  of  the  thirty-four  subjects, 
give  their  average  time  for  the  W.  type  less  than  that  for  their 
R.  type.  Thirteen  of  the  thirty-four  subjects  give  their  average 
time  for  the  W.  type  longer  than  that  for  the  R.  type.  Five  of 
the  thirty-'four  are  unable  to  be  compared.  These  facts  of  time 
make  more  certain  the  correctness  of  our  modification  introduced 
in  the  preceding  paragraph.  Again,  we  may  say,  when  the  re- 
sults are  considered  in  relation  to  each  other,  that  neither  the 
number  of  subjects  favoring  the  R.  type  over  the  W.  type,  nor 
the  number  giving  shorter  times  for  their  successful  responses 
with  the  R.  type  than  with  the  W.  type,  is  indicative  of  any 
deleterious  effect  of  retroactive  inhibition. 

EXPERIMENT  XIII 

The  present  experiment  is  a  departure  from  the  previous  ex- 
periments, both  as  to  method  and  as  to  apparatus.  We  may 


58  /.  EDGAR  DECAMP 

designate  the  method  as  of  the  Reconstruction  type.  The  ap- 
paratus consisted  of  a  chess-board,  five  distinct  chess-men  (a 
pawn,  a  knight,  a  bishop,  a  rook,  and  a  queen),  a  piece  of  card- 
board 51  x  62  cm.,  and  a  stop-watch. 

The  chess-'board  was  placed  upon  a  table.  The  subject,  Z.  B., 
sat  at  one  side  of  the  table;  the  operator  stood  at  the  other. 
The  operator  placed  five  chess-men  upon  different  squares  of 
the  chess-board,  and  took  a  record  of  their  position.  During 
this  arrangement  the  piece  of  cardboard  rested  on  the  table,  on 
the  long  side  as  a  base,  between  the  chess-board  and  the  subject's 
eyes,  preventing  the  subject  from  seeing  the  position  of  the 
chess-men.  The  cardboard  was  raised  and  the  subject  allowed 
fifteen  seconds  to  study  the  different  positions  of  the  different 
men.  After  the  fifteen  seconds  exposure,  the  cardboard 
was  replaced  in  its  previous  position  and  the  men 
were  removed  by  the  operator  and  placed  in  a  convenient 
place  for  the  subject  to  get.  The  cardboard  was  then 
laid  over  the  face  of  the  chess-board.  The  three  minutes 
immediately  following  the  closure  of  the  exposing  process, 
found  disposition  in  two  general  types,  a  rest  (R.)>  and  a 
work  (W.)  type.  The  R.  type  consisted  in  the  subject's 
doing  nothing  for  the  three  minutes.  The  W.  type  consisted 
in  the  subject's  working  two  minutes  and  resting  one  minute. 
For  the  first  eight  days  the  work  consisted  in  the  addition  of 
columns  of  figures;  for  the  last  four  days  simple  arithmetical 
problems  were  used,  a  new  one  being  given  each  required  time. 
Three  minutes  after  the  end  of  the  exposure  process,  the  card- 
board was  removed  from  the  chess-board  for  the  reconstruction, 
by  the  subject,  of  the  previous  positions  occupied  by  the  different 
chess-men.  The  time  required  by  the  subject  for  the  recon- 
struction, was  taken  with  the  stop-watch.  After  the  recon- 
struction the  cardboard  was  replaced  in  its  position  between  the 
subject  and  the  chessboard,  and  the  reconstructed  position  of 
the  men  recorded  by  the  operator.  The  experiment  was  then 
repeated.  At  least  three  minutes  rest  was  allowed  the  subject 
between  the  closure  of  the  reconstruction  and  the  beginning  of 
the  second  experiment.  On  any  experiment  day  six  tests  were 


A  STUDY  OF  RETROACTIVE  INHIBITION  59 

made.  The  R.  and  W.  types  alternated  on  any  particular  day. 
The  first  three  days  began  with  the  R.  type,  the  next  three  began 
with  the  W.  type,  the  next  three  with  the  R.  type,  and  the  last 
three  with  the  W.  type.  The  experiment  continued  twelve  ex- 
periment days,  giving  thirty-six  tests  for  the  R.  and  thirty-six 
tests  for  the  W.  type. 

The  errors  in  the  reconstructed  forms,  and  the  time,  were 
computed  and  tabulated.  The  error  value  for  any  chess-man, 
in  the  reconstructed  form,  was  obtained,  speaking  mathematically, 
by  adding  the  differences  between  the  ordinates  and  the 
abscissae  in  the  two  cases,  chess-board  squares  considered  as 
units.  To  illustrate,  a  piece  originally  placed  at  b4  (German 
chess  notation),  and  in  the  reconstruction,  placed  at  g2,  would 
give  rise  to  an  error  value  of  5  +  2  °r  7  f°r  the  particular  piece. 
The  errors  for  the  five  men  were  totalled,  and  this  total  was 
considered  the  error  value  of  the  test. 

The  results  of  the  experiment  follow  in  Table  XIX : 

TABLE  XIX 

/ — R.    Type — i  , — W.  Type — » 

E.  T.  E.  T. 

i R                  31  265  38  192 

2 R                  13  135  54  232 

3 R                    33  220  26  197 

4 W                  29  209  24  117 

5 W                  34  146  38  259 

6 W                   59  142  44  169 

7 R                    43  294  20  152 

8 R                  24  92  27  102 

9 R                  36  204  41  150 

10 W                 29  174  28  194 

ii W                 18  191  41  209 

12 W                 24  151  41  279 

Totals 373  2223  422  2252 

Av 10.4  67.4  1 1.7  66.2 

M.v 5.6  6.0 

The  first  column  gives  the  number  of  the  experiment  day  and 
the  type  of  test  with  which  the  work  began  on  that  day.  Under 
R.  Type  and  W.  Type  are  given  the  errors,  in  sub-column  E., 
and  time,  under  sub-column  T. ;  the  figures  given  represent  the 
total  of  the  three  tests  of  similar  type  for  the  experiment  day 
opposite  which  the  particular  figures  appear. 

The  difference  between  the  averages  for  the  two  cases  is  so 


6o  /.   EDGAR  DECAMP 

small  as  to  be  wholly  non-significant,  while  the  m.  v.  is  non- 
indicative. 

In  the  reconstruction  here  required  of  the  subject,  we  are 
dealing  not  merely  with  the  impressibility  aspect,  but  with  the 
subject's  ability  to  hold  in  mind  an  ordered  system.  This  re- 
tention of  systematic  order  is  doubtless  one  of  man's  last 
acquirements  in  his  mental  evolution,  and  considered  from  the 
aspect  of  perseveration,  appears  to  be  one  of  the  first  to  drop 
out.  Consequently  we  might  expect  retroactive  inhibition,  as- 
suming its  existence,  so  much  the  more  to  manifest  itself  when 
dealing  with  this  apparently  unstable  mental  phenomenon.  From 
the  results  at  hand  such,  however,  is  not  the  case. 

G.     GENERAL  DISCUSSION 

In  dealing  with  the  individual  experiments  we  have  par- 
tially discussed  the  results  obtained.  A  systematic  unification 
remains  to  be  accomplished. 

Knowing  the  conditions  set  by  an  experimenter,  his  results 
place  all  who  care  to  consider  them  upon  a  fairly  equal  basis. 
The  domain  of  results  gives  way  to  that  of  interpretation, 
from  which  standpoint  divergent  lines  may  be  taken  by  differ- 
ent interpreters.  It  is  in  this  field  of  interpretation  that  the 
scientist,  though  in  a  sense  "driven  by  the  facts",  in  another 
sense,  "moulds  his  own  laws  and  makes  his  own  conclusions." 

To  the  writer  it  seems  unquestionable,  from  the  results  pre- 
sented in  this  paper,  that  too  much  stress  has  hitherto  been  laid 
upon  the  effect  of  retroactive  inhibition.  That  there  is  such  a 
thing  as  retroactive  inhibition  we  are  not  wholly  prepared  to 
deny.  Our  results  seem  to  justify  the  statement — and  this  is 
our  main  THESIS — That  retroactive  inhibition  plays  a  significant 
part  in  influencing  the  recall  of  nonsense  syllables,  appears  ex- 
ceedingly doubtful.  The  tentativeness  of  our  position  follows 
necessarily  from  the  considerations  already  presented.  Ex- 
haustive experiments  are  necessary  before  generalities  can  be 
indulged  in  if,  indeed,  generalities  are  ever  permissible  in  deal- 
ing with  mental  phenomena. 

It  may  be  suggested  that  our  results,   and  consequent  con- 


A  STUDY  OF  RETROACTIVE  INHIBITION  61 

elusions,  conflict  with  the  accepted  view  of  the  working  of  the 
Perseveration  Tendency,  which,  indeed,  is  one  of  the  essential 
units  upon  which  the  theoretical  argument  for  the  existence  and 
explanation  of  retroactive  inhibition  is  based.  Muller  and 
Pilzecker  appear  to  have  thought  of  the  nature  of  the  perse  vera- 
tion  tendency  as  a  kind  of  after-discharge — a  continued  ac- 
tivity— of  the  nerve  elements  following  any  learning  activity. 
The  physiological  work  of  Sherrington1  on  the  spinal  cord  and 
the  work  on  inhibition  by  Shepard2  indicate  that  the  after^ 
discharge  of  nerve  elements  is  definitely  inhibited  by  any  neural 
activity  taking  place  during  this  after-discharge.  From  the 
standpoint  of  the  greater  value  of  divided  repetitions  as  com- 
pared with  accumulated  repetitions  the  perseveration  tendency 
as  such  seems  definitely  established.  Our  results  leave  retro- 
active inhibition  questionable.  The  logical  consequence,  then, 
is  that  the  nature  of  the  perseveration  tendency,  which  leads 
to  the  setting  of  associations,  is  not  that  of  an  after-discharge. 
Again,  it  seems  that  hitherto  emphasis  has  been  laid  upon 
retroactive  inhibition  as  an  inhibition  presenting  a  single  as- 
pect— and  that  inhibitory.  The  work  of  both  Sherrington  and 
Shepard  seems  to  indicate  that  inhibition  is  not  a  single-phased 
process.  According  to  these  investigators  two  processes  giving 
rise  to  inhibition  mutually  inhibit  each  other;  neither  can  inhibit 
the  other  without  itself  being  subject  to  inhibition.  If  retro- 
active inhibition  is  to  be  conceived  as  according  with  this  idea 
of  the  double  aspect  of  inhibition  in  general,  then  we  should 
expect  the  learning  of  a  Hauptreihe  to  inhibit  the  learning  of 
a  Nachreihe  as  well  as  the  learning  of  a  Nachreihe  to  inhibit 
the  preceding  Hauptreihe.  Consequently,  against  the  conception 
of  retroactive  inhibition  as  of  a  single  aspect' — and  this  is  prob- 
ably the  conception  of  Muller  and  Pilzecker — there  may  be 
urged  two  arguments,  (i)  the  difficulty  of  the  conception  of 
a  one-sided  inhibition,  and  (2)  the  results  of  the  present  work, 
which  do  not  necessarily  require  the  assumption  of  retroactive 
inhibition  at  all. 

1  The  Integrative  Action  of  the  Nervous  System,  1906. 

2  Psychological  Review,  Vol.  XX,  No.  4,  July,  1913. 


62  /.  EDGAR  DECAMP 

We  have  tended  to  ascribe  most  of  the  differences  between 
the  comparison  and  the  main  series  to  chance  variation.  It 
may  be  claimed — and  rightly — that  chance  variation  should  give 
evidence  against  as  often  as  for  retroactive  inhibition.  Con- 
sidering our  experiments  in  their  totality,  the  majority  of  them 
slightly  iavor  a  trace  of  retroactive  inhibition.  Whether  this 
tendency  of  the  majority  is  itself  a  variation  further  experi- 
ments may  decide.  At  present,  in  the  opinion  of  the  writer, 
the  validity  of  retroactive  inhibition  remains  more  or  less 
questionable.  If  we  assume  a  slight  amount  of  retroactive  in- 
hibition it  may  still  be  explained  as  due  to  a  tendency  of  the 
work  to  block  the  after-discharge  of  the  just  learned  syllables. 
The  nature  of  this  blocking  is  a  difficult  question. 

For  the  small  amount  of  retroactive  inhibition  granted  I 
may  be  permitted  to  suggest  an  explanation  analagous  to  that 
existing  for  the  transference  of  training.  From  the  neuro- 
logical standpoint,  in  the  learning  of  a  series  of  syllables,  we 
may  assume  that  a  certain  group  of  synapses,  nerve  cells,  nerve 
'paths,  centres,  etc.,  are  involved.  Immediately  after  the  learn- 
ing process  the  after-discharge  continues  for  a  short  time,  tending 
to  set  the  associations  between  the  just  learned  syllables.  Any 
mental  activity  engaged  in  during  this  after-discharge,  involv- 
ing or  partially  involving  the  same  neurological  group,  tends, 
more  or  less,  to  block  the  after-discharge,  and  gives  rise  to 
retroactive  inhibition.  Engagement  in  any  mental  activity, 
involving  a  new — so  'far  as  it  is  new — group  of  synapses, 
neurones,  etc.,  would  allow  the  setting  process  of  the  just  excited 
group  to  proceed  unhindered.  The  effect  of  retroactive  inhibi- 
tion would  vary  directly  as  the  relative  identity  of  the  neuro- 
logical groups  concerned.  It  appears  exceedingly  plausible  that 
any  given  group  involved  in  learning  nonsense  syllables  is  a 
relatively  restricted  and,  more  or  less,  isolated  one,  scarcely 
involving  the  more  intricate  ramifications  of  the  higher  associa- 
tion paths  and  centres,  and  hence  much  less  liable  to  be  in- 
fluenced by  mental  activity,  involving  the  complexity  of  the 
higher  centres  and  processes.  Upon  this  view  we  should  expect 
retroactive  inhibition  to  appear  more  readily  where  material 


A  STUDY  OF  RETROACTIVE  INHIBITION  63 

similar  to  that  learned  is  used  for  the  interpolated  work.  Where 
the  learning  of  nonsense  syllables  is  followed  by  work  of  a 
different  nature — a  mathematical  problem,  for  instance — the 
relative  number  of  identical  elements  of  the  two  groups  may 
be  so  few,  owing  to  the  complexity  of  man's  neurological 
system,  that  no  appreciable  retroactive  effect  is  elicited. 

The  neurological  explanation  just  presented  seems  sufficient 
where  the  deleterious  effect  of  the  work  is  slight  but  appears 
inadequate  for  the  results  of  Miiller  and  Pilzecker  which  were 
obtained  by  using  additional  nonsense  syllables  as  the  interpolated 
work.  To  the  writer  it  appears  that  Effectual  Inhibition, 
occasioned  by  merely  increasing  the  number  of  syllables,  may 
play  the  decisive  part  in  lessening  the  percentage  of  successful 
responses  in  the  recall  of  a  Hauptreihe  which  has  been  followed 
by  the  learning  of  a  Nachreihe.  Probably  in  this  way  is  to  be 
explained  the  following  quotation  from  Eleanor  C.  McC.  Gam- 
ble's "A  Study  in  Memorizing  Various  Materials  by  the 
Reconstruction  Method."1 

"With  G.  retroactive  inhibition  was  obvious.  Each  series 
[i.e.  of  smells,  colors,  nonsense  syllables]  was  remembered 
fairly  well  until  the  next  was  given.  That  is  to  say,  the  subject 
could  name  hours  afterwards  the  members  of  the  last  series 
given,  and  the  associations  involved  would  remain  almost  un- 
disturbed for  weeks.  But  as  soon  as  a  new  series  was-  given, 
the  eraser  (the  subject  actually  visualized  a  blackboard  eraser 
in  this  connection)  was  drawn  over  the  old  series.  A  'smudge' 
might  remain  to  make  the  new  series  illegible  but  the  old 
one  could  no  longer  be  read  off." 

In  the  opinion  of  the  writer  a  similar  explanation  applies 
to  Ebbinghaus'2  attribution  to  retroactive  inhibition  of  the  ex- 
traordinary increase  in  the  number  of  repetitions  necessary  for 
memorizing  lists  of  syllables  as  the  "memory  span"  is  exceeded. 
To  quote, 

"Die  einzelnen  Associationen  beeintrachtigen  und  lockern 
sich  immer  wieder  durch  ruckwirkende  Hemmung,  und  die 

1  Psychological  Review,  Monograph  Sup.  No.  43,  1909,  p.  138. 
3  Grundziige  der  Psychologic,  1902,  S.  652. 


64  /.   EDGAR  DECAMP 

Herstellung  einer  bestimmten  Festigkeit  erfordert  mithin  einen 
immer  grosseren  Arbeitsaufwand  zur  Uberwindung  dieser 
Stoning,  je  hoher  die  Zahl  der  hemmenden  und  gehemmten 
Glieder  sich  belauft." 

But,  in  the  light  of  the  present  experiments,  the  writer  does 
not  feel  disposed  towards  accepting,  as  a  general  conclusion, 
even  a  greater  difference  when  a  consideration  of  pictures  is 
substituted  for  the  Nachreihe.  There  seems  little  doubt  that 
cross-multiplication  involves  the  expenditure  of  at  least  as 
much' — and  very  likely  more — mental  energy  as  the  contempla- 
tion of  landscape  pictures.  With  the  use  of  such  work  the  only 
one  of  our  experiments  particularly  favoring  the  results  of 
Miiller  and  Pilzecker  is  Experiment  III,  and  in  that  experiment 
the  results  are  much  less  "striking"  than  those  obtained  by 
Miiller  and  Pilzecker.  The  same  subject,  however,  in  Experi- 
ment I  gave  the  two  lowest  points  (of  16  points)  of  the  success 
curve  with  R.  15,  W.  o,  and  in  Experiment  IV  gave  even  more 
successes  with  R.  2,  W.  13  than  with  R.  15,  W.  o,  though  this 
latter  point  can  have  no  particular  weight  against  the  results 
of  Miiller  and  Pilzecker,  since  their  work  with  pictures  ex- 
tended no  farther  than  two  minutes  after  learning. 

It  might  be  argued  that  retroactive  inhibition,  expending  its 
effect  upon  the  rapidly  descending  part  of  the  forgetting  curve 
(as  given  by  Ebbinghaus1)  may  influence  the  relative  number  of 
successes  relatively  more  after  a  lapse  of  eight  minutes  than 
after  a  lapse  of  fifteen  minutes,  thereby  partially  harmonizing 
our  results  with  nonsense  syllables  with  those  of  Miiller  and 
Pilzecker.  Admitting  the  validity  of  some  such  argument,  it 
appears  extremely  insufficient  to  explain  the  differences  ob- 
tained. Even  'from  the  standpoint  of  reaction  time  we  fail 
to  agree  with  the  German  investigators.  Some  of  our  experi- 
ments harmonize  with  their  time  results,  while  others  oppose. 
Seemingly,  then,  the  only  course  open  to  us  is  to  place  the 
results  of  their  Versuchreihe  35  in  some  such  class  as  "Happy 
Accident,"  or  question  their  saliency  upon  the  basis  of 
unnaturalness. 
1  Vber  das  Geddchtniss,  1885,  p.  203. 


A  STUDY  OF  RETROACTIVE  INHIBITION  65 

It  can  scarcely  be  maintained  or  denied  that  a  subject's 
tendency  or  non-tendency  towards  the  formation  of  sense  asso- 
ciations is  a  determinant  of  the  appearance  or  non-appearance 
of  results  indicative  of  retroactive  inhibition.  Subject  B., 
whose  tendency  in  this  respect  was  marked  in  the  preliminary 
experiments,  though  not  particularly  noticeable  in  the  present 
work,  is  the  only  one  giving  fairly  unquestionable  results  in 
favor  of  retroactive  inhibition,  and  from  the  introspective  side, 
it  will  be  remembered,  this  subject  stated  that  work  had  no 
influence:  if  he  had  a  syllable,  he  had  it.  This,  to  be  sure, 
had  no  significance  further  than  that  the  subject  noticed  no 
particular  influence  of  the  work.  Subject  A.  of  Experiment  X, 
whose  tendency  towards  the  formation  of  sense  connections  be- 
tween the  syllables  was  well  known  previous  to  the  experiment, 
and  appeared  throughout  the  experiment,  gave  results  inhar- 
monious with  the  assumed  deleterious  effect  of  retroactive  in- 
hibition. Our  other  subjects,  it  may  be  said,  had  no  particular 
tendency  in  this  respect,  and  not  one  of  these  gave  results 
unquestionably  in  favor  of  the  existence  of  retroactive  inhibi- 
tion. Argumentatively,  from  one  standpoint  at  least,  if  retro- 
active inhibition  is  general  in  its  effect,  it  would  seem  that  of 
the  two  classes  of  subjects,  those  subjects  possessing  no  par- 
ticular tendency  towards  sense  associations  should  show  greater 
evidence  of  the  presence  of  retroactive  inhibition. 

Doubtless  it  will  have  been  noticed  that  the  discussion  has 
almost  wholly  neglected  the  results  of  Experiment  VIII.  As 
was  mentioned  previously,  the  fatigue  element  complicates  these 
results  and  forbids  their  consideration  in  favor  of  retroactive 
inhibition. 

Professor  Pillsbury  has  indicated1  the  analogy  between  retro- 
active inhibition  and  the  retrograde  annesia  of  the  psychiatrist. 
In  the  case  of  the  strong  blow  or  emotional  shock,  it  may  be 
assumed  that  the  effect  is  a  general  one  and  that  the  bonds, 
connecting  the  neurological  groups,  acting  at  the  time  with 
the  remaining  neurological  groups,  are  severed.  The  events 
represented  by  this  now  isolated  group  are  forgotten  in  the 

1  Essentials  of  Psychology,  1913,  p.  196. 


66  7.  EDGAR  DECAMP 

sense  that  they  are  not  easily  reached  from  the  remaining  neu- 
rological groups.  That  they  are  not  wholly  lost  to  the  individual 
is  shown  by  the  fact  that  they  may  later  return  to  consciousness, 
or,  under  suitable  conditions,  be  reached.  Interpolated  work, 
it  may  be  said,  does  not  tend  towards  any  such  isolation  of  a 
group  which  has  just  been  excited.  Consequently,  the  fact 
that  our  results  are  not  similar  to  those  obtained  by  the 
psychiatrist,  does  not  necessarily  argue  against  their  validity. 
The  number  of  partial  responses  is  so  small  that  we  make 
no  attempt  to  draw,  from  their  distribution,  any  conclusion 
with  respect  to  retroactive  inhibition.  We  may  mention  that  the 
average  time  for  syllables,  two  letters  correct,  is  much  longer 
than  that  for  wholly  successful  responses,  and  the  average  time 
for  syllables,  one  letter  correct,  is  still  longer.  Comparative  re- 
sults of  a  single  experiment,  Experiment  IX  (a),  will  suffice  to 
show  this  difference,  Table  XX: 

TABLE  XX 
Work  0123456 

S3  7039(52)      6756(42)       5625(45)       6548(48)       5967(50)       7773(50)       5706(43) 

S2  11982(8)      15994(5)        8843(11)     13506(15)     i495i(7)      10033(11)      8596(10) 

St 19610(11)     19480(9)      22849(10)     23044(12)     17684(10)     21981(14)     17406(15) 

The  parenthesized  figures  represent  the  number  of  responses 
for  which  the  adjacent  figures  are  the  average  time. 

H.     SUMMARY  AND  CONCLUSIONS 

The  present  work  grew  out  of  experiments  conducted  by 
Muller  and  Pilzecker,  whose  results  showed  decided  evidence  in 
favor  of  the  deleterious  influence,  upon  learned  nonsense 
syllables,  of  learning  a  second  series  of  syllables,  or  of  observing 
and  picturing  to  the  operator  landscape  pictures  immediately 
after  the  learning  of  the  first  series  of  syllables.  Their  experi- 
ment with  pictures  as  interpolated  work,  showed  slightly  more 
decided  results  in  favor  of  retroactive  inhibition  than  did  the 
experiments  with  a  second  series  of  syllables  for  the  interpolated 
work. 

Our  work  embraces  a  series  of  thirteen  experiments,  extend- 
ing over  a  period  of  two  school  years  and  one  summer. 


A  STUDY  OF  RETROACTIVE  INHIBITION  67 

The  apparatus  consisted  of  a  modified  form  of  the  Wirth 
card-exposure  apparatus,  Hipp  chronoscope,  and  subsidiary 
apparatus.  In  Experiment  XIII  a  chess-board,  men,  and  addi- 
tional apparatus  were  used.  Experiments  I  to  XI  and  XIII  were 
extended  experiments  with  seven  subjects.  In  Experiment  XII 
thirty-four  subjects  were  involved. 

Series  of  fourteen  nonsense  syllables  (save  in  Experiment 
XIII)  were  repeated  in  pairs  a  certain  number  of  times.  On  each 
experiment  day  two  series,  an  A  and  a  B,  were  learned  and 
tested,  an  interval  of  at  least  five  minutes  intervening  between  the 
work  with  the  two  series.  The  purpose  of  the  experiments 
was  a  comparison  between  syllables  learned  with  no  interpolated 
work  following,  and  syllables  learned  with  work — usually  cross- 
multiplication — variously  distributed  within  the  interval  im- 
mediately following  the  learning  process.  The  order  of  the 
syllables  was  changed,  and  after  a  definite  interval,  the  first 
syllable  of  each  measure  was  tested  for  the  associated  syllable. 
The  response,  reaction  time,  and  introspections  of  the  subject, 
were  recorded.  Experiment  XIII  approached  the  problem  from 
the  standpoint  of  reconstruction  of  positions  of  chess-men, 
where  the  studying,  by  the  subject,  of  such  positions  was  fol- 
lowed in  one  case  by  work,  in  another,  by  rest. 

Experiment  III  is  the  only  experiment  giving  results  char- 
acteristically favoring  the  assumption  of  retroactive  inhibition. 
The  other  experiments — Experiment  VIII  excepted — including 
our  most  extended  one — Experiment  IX< — do  not  particularly 
speak  for  the  existence  of  retroactive  inhibition,  either  with  re- 
spect to  the  number  of  successes,  or  the  reaction  time  of  the  suc- 
cessful responses.  Experiment  XII,  upon  two  counts,  adds  de- 
cided weight  in  favor  of  the  non-existence  of  retroactive  inhibition. 
Experiment  XI  militates  decidedly  against  the  assumption  of 
retroactive  inhibition.  Hence  we  must  needs  suggest  that  the 
influence  of  retroactive  inhibition  is  fairly  unimportant  and 
has  been  given  too  great  prominence  among  psychological 
principles. 

We  may  suggest  the  following  conclusions  as  indicated,  if 
not  wholly  proven,  by  the  results  of  our  experiments : 


68  /.  EDGAR  DECAMP 

1.  That  retroactive  inhibition  plays  a  significant  part  in  in- 
fluencing   the    recall    of    nonsense    syllables,    either    from    the 
standpoint  of  the  number  of  successes,   or  the  length  of  the 
reaction  time,  or  both,  appears  exceedingly  doubtful. 

2.  No  positive  introspective  evidence  appeared  in   favor  of 
retroactive  inhibition. 

3.  With    inexperienced    subjects    no    evidence    appeared    in 
favor  of  retroactive  inhibition,   either  from  a  combination  of 
their   results,    or    from   a   consideration    of    their    comparative 
results. 

4.  A  neurological  explanation  of  a  slight  amount  of  retro- 
active  inhibition   is   tentatively   offered;   Retroactive   inhibition 
may  present  itself  where  relatively  identical  or  partially  identical 
groups  of  nerve  centres,  neurones,  synapses,  etc.,  are  involved 
in  learning  the  series  of  syllables  and  in  the  interpolated  mental 
activity.     As  the  neurone  groups  have  relatively  less  and  less 
in  common,  retroactive  inhibition  may  manifest  itself  less  and 
less. 

5.  From  the  standpoint  of  the  position  occupied  in  the  learn- 
ing,  the  seventh  and  first  measures   usually  gave  the   largest 
number  of  successful  responses. 

6.  The  B  series  of  syllables  possessed  a  slight  disadvantage 
as  compared  with  the  A  series,  probably  due  to  a  slight  fatigue 
effect  occasioned  by  the  A  series. 

7.  Different  subjects  showed  no  marked  individual  differences 
with  respect  to  the  manifestation  of  the  effect  of  retroactive 
inhibition. 

The  author  wishes  to  acknowledge  his  especial  indebtedness 
to  Prof.  W.  B.  Pillsbury  and  Asst.  Prof.  J.  F.  Shepard,  under 
whose  directions  this  work  was  carried  out. 


A  STUDY  OF  RETROACTIVE  INHIBITION  69 

BIBLIOGRAPHY 

EBBINGHAUS,  H.,  1902.     Grundziige  der  Psychologic. 

> 1885.     tiber  das  Gedachtniss. 

LADD,   G.   T.,   and   WOODWORTH,   R.    S.,    1911.     Elements   of 

Physiological  Psychology. 
McC.  GAMBLE,  E.  A.,  1909.     A  Study  in  Memorizing  Various 

Materials    by    the    Reconstruction    Method. 

Psychological   Review,    Monograph   No.   43, 

Vol.  X.,  No.  4. 

MEUMANN,  E.,  1912.     The  Psychology  of  Learning.     Trans- 
lation by  J.  W.  Baird,  1913. 
MULLER,    G.    E.,    and   PILZECKER,    A.,    1900.      Experimented 

Beitrage  zur  Lehre  vom  Gedachtniss. 
MULLER,  G.  E.,  1912.     tiber  Wiedererkennen  und  riickwirkende 

Hemmung.     Bericht  iiber  den   V .  Kongress 

fur  experiment elle  Psychologie. 

MYERS,  C.  S.,  1911.     A  Text-book  of  Experimental  Psychology. 
PILLSBURY,  W.  B.,  1913.     Essentials  of  Psychology. 
SHEPARD,  J.  F.,  and  FOGELSONGER,  H.  M.,  1913.     Studies  in 

Association    and    Inhibition.      Psychological 

Review,  Vol.  XX.,  No.  4. 


BF 

1 

P69 

v.19 


Psychological  monographs:   gen- 
eral and  applied 


PLEASE  DO  NOT  REMOVE 
SLIPS  FROM  THIS  POCKET 


UNIVERSITY  OF  TORONTO 
LIBRARY