Skip to main content

Full text of "The Psychology Of Ego Involvements"

See other formats


TEXT  FLY  WITHIN 
THE  BOOK  ONLY 


< 

00 


64361 


OSMANIA   UNIVERSITY  LIBRARY 


Call  No.  /  3  /  /  a  h  I    Accession  No.  ^  | 
Author 


This  bookshoul 
last  marked  below. 


rettrfned  6n  or  before  the  date 


The  Psychology  of 
Ego -Involvements 


SQCIAL  ATTITUDES 

fr 
IDENTIFICATIONS 


MUZAFER  SHERIF 

• 

HADLEY  CANTRIL 

Princeton  University 


New  Yor*:  JOHN  WILEY  &  SONS,  Inc. 
London:  CHAPMAN  &  HALL,  Limited 


COPYRIGHT,  1947 

BY 
JOHN  WILEY  &  SONS,  INC. 

AU  Rights  Reserved 

This  book  or  any  part  thereof  must  not 
be  reproduced  in  any  form  without 
the  written  permission  of  the  publisher. 

SECOND  PRINTING,    NOVEMBER,    1947 


PRINTED  IN  THE  UNITED  STATES  OF  AMERICA 


PREFACE 

In  this  book  on  the  psychology  of  ego-involvements  we  are 
attacking  a  problem  which  it  seems  crucial  to  solve  if  we  are  ever 
to  acquire  a  scientifically  defensible  account  of  man's  relationship 
to  the  world  around  him  with  all  its  natural  laws,  its  technological 
developments,  and  its  social  products. 

Here  we  are  only  able  to  sketch  in  bold  relief  what  appears  to 
be  the  psychology  of  ego-involvements.  Much  more  must  be  added 
later  to  complete  the  account.  But,  as  we  point  out  in  the  first 
chapter,  it  seems  to  us  that  the  broad  outline  presented  here  will 
accommodate  this  further  work  without  any  major  alterations. 

Evidence  from  a  wide  variety  of  sources,  including  studies  from 
the  experimental  laboratory,  investigations  of  everyday  life  be- 
havior, public-opinion  surveys,  observational  studies  of  children  and 
adolescents,  sociological  data,  and  field  material  of  anthropologists 
and  ethnologists,  has  almost  fallen  into  place  of  its  own  accord. 
Although  we  have  cited  rather  extensive  references,  our  job  has 
been  more  one  of  selection  than  accumulation  of  data:  in  nearly 
every  chapter,  we  have  had  to  satisfy  ourselves  with  the  inclusion 
of  only  a  few  of  the  many  reports,  observations,  or  experiments 
that  might  have  been  mentioned.  Those  who  are  familiar  with 
the  various  areas  we  have  touched  on  will  know  the  vast  literature 
it  is  possible  to  tap. 

The  bulk  of  chapters  2,  3,  and  4  were  first  published  in  the  Psy- 
chological Review  in  November  1945  and  January  1946.  These 
articles  have  been  slightly  elaborated  here.  The  material  is  re- 
published  with  the  permission  of  the  American  Psychological  As- 
sociation. The  writers  acknowledge  the  permissions  granted  by 
the  various  publishers  and  authors  for  the  quotations  used.  Great 
care  has  been  taken  in  each  instance  to  provide  bibliographical 
references  by  means  of  which  the  reader  can  identify  the  author 
and  publisher  to  whom  credit  is  thereby  given. 

A  State  Department  fellowship  to  Princeton  University  for 


vi  PREFACE 

Shcrif  and  a  contribution  from  the  Marshall  Field  Foundation  to 
Princeton  University,  enabling  Cantril  to  be  relieved  of  some  teach- 
ing duties  and  allowing  us  bibliographical  and  secretarial  assist- 
ance, combined  to  make  it  possible  for  us  to  work  closely  together. 
We  acknowledge  this  assistance  most  gratefully. 

Carolyn  Wood  Sherif  has  worked  with  us  since  late  1945.  She 
has  greatly  aided  us  in  selecting  some  of  the  material  and  in  pre- 
paring certain  chapters. 

We  gratefully  acknowledge  the  help  of  Drs.  Harold  and  Mary 
Cover  Jones  of  the  Institute  of  Child  Welfare  of  the  University  of 
California  for  allowing  us  to  go  over  some  of  their  unpublished 
material  on  adolescence. 

We  are  indebted  to  Donald  Stauff er  and  Whitney  J.  Gates  of  the 
Princeton  faculty  for  their  apt  suggestions  of  certain  of  the  literary 
passages  cited  in  chapter  13.  Mildred  Strunk  also  assisted  us  in  the 
selection  of  examples  for  this  chapter. 

Elizabeth  V.  Deyo  has  ably  managed  many  of  the  details  con- 
nected with  our  work  and  has  prepared  the  manuscript  for  pub- 
lication, 

Dr.  Herbert  S.  Langfeld  made  a  number  of  editorial  suggestions 
which  improved  the  manuscript. 

M.S. 
H.C. 

Princeton,  N.  J. 
June  24, 1946. 


CONTENTS 

CHAPTER  PAGE 

1  INTRODUCTION  1 

2  THE  PROBLEM  OF  ATTITUDES:  UNIFIED  9 
Recent  Attitude  Studies  and  Methodological  Considerations 

— A  Characterization  of  Attitudes. 

3  SOME  EXPERIMENTAL  FACTS  CONCERNING  THE  BASIS  OF 

ATTITUDES  29 

Frames  of  Reference  in  Relation  to  Structured  and  Unstruc- 
tured Stimulus  Situations— Social  Factors  in  Laboratory  Situ- 
ations. 

4  THE  ATTITUDE  FORMULATION  EXTENDED  TO  CONCRETE 

SITUATIONS  60 

Experiments  Using  Everyday  Life  Material — Stereotypes — 
Political  Attitudes— Panics— -Social  Movements— Frame  of 
Reference  in  Some  Recent  Economic  Works. 

5  THE  PROBLEM  AND  A  GENERAL  CHARACTERIZATION  OF  EGO- 

INVOLVEMENTS  92 

Misuse  of  the  Concept  of  "Ego"— Attitudes  as  Main  Consti- 
tuents of  Ego — Ego  a  Genetic  Formation — Ego  Not  a  Fixed 
Entity — Ego-striving  is  Not  Instinctive:  No  Innate  Ego 
Drives. 

6  EXPERIMENTS  ON  EGO-INVOLVEMENTS  117 
Ego-involvements  in  Laboratory  Tasks — Ego-involvements  as 

a  Set  for  Learning — Ego-involvements  Shown  in  the  Intensity 
of  Attitudes — Status  and  Class  Identification  as  Ego-involve- 
ments—Some  Recent  Reviews  of  Experimental  Literature. 

7  THE  GENETIC  FORMATION  OF  THE  EGO  156 
A  Review  of  Some  Studies  on  Ego  Development— The  Ef- 
fects of  Certain  Social  Factors  on  Ego  Formation. 

8  RE-FORMATION  OF  THE  EGO  IN  ADOLESCENCE  199 
The  Problem  of  Adolescence  in  a  Sociological  and  Psycho- 
logical Setting— Adolescence  in  Different  Cultures  and  Times. 

vii 


viii  CONTENTS 

CHAPTER  PAGE 

9  RE-FORMATION  OF  THE  EGO  IN  ADOLESCENCE  (Continued)  222 
The  Developing  Ego  Rendered  Unstable  and,  at  Times,  Criti- 
cal—The Adolescents'  Reactions  and  Efforts  to  Re-establish 
Themselves  to  Some  Degree  of  Stability— The  Effects  of  Age- 
mate  Reference  Groups  and  Membership  Groups  in  the  Re- 
formation of  the  Ego-attitudes— Some  Implications  of  the 
Foregoing  Material  for  Our  Problem. 

10  EGO-INVOLVEMENTS  AND  IDENTIFICATIONS  IN  GROUP  SITUA- 

TIONS 280 

The  Structural  Properties  of  Groups— Individual  Members  as 
Influenced  by  Group  Norms  and  Identifications— Some  Psy- 
chological Leads— Some  Verifications  of  the  Effects  of  Group 
Situations. 

11  EGO-INVOLVEMENTS  IN  CONCRETE  SOCIAL  SITUATIONS   >       348 
"The  Apparel  Oft  Proclaims  the  Man"— We  See  Ourselves  in 

the  Movies— "What's  in  a  Name?"— The  Martyr— Ego-in- 
volvements Determined  by  Situation— Status  within  Groups 
—Job  Satisfaction. 

12  BREAKDOWNS  OF  THE  EGO  386 
Dissociation  of  the  Ego  from  the  Body— The  Ego  May  Tem- 
porarily Dissolve— Breakdowns  of  the  Ego  Under  Extreme 
Deprivation— The  Ego  May  Break  Down  if  Established 
Norms  Collapse— The  Ego  May  Regress,  Dissociate,  or  Be- 
come Re-formed  Under  Extreme  Situations  of  Stress— Ego- 
breakdowns  in  Abnormal  and  Pathological  Cases. 

13  SOME  ILLUSTRATIONS  FROM  LITERATURE  437 

14  THE  EGO  IN  PSYCHOANALYSIS  461 
\Freud's  Formulations— Where  Freudian  Theory  Leads  in 

Social  Psychology— Modifications  with  Cultural  Emphasis- 
Why  Psychoanalytic  Formulations  Must  Be  Rejected- 
Summary. 

NAME  INDEX  509 

SUBJECT  INDEX  516 


CHAPTER  1 
INTRODUCTION 

Among  other  problems  brought  into  sharp  focus  by  the  impact 
of  momentous  events  and  changes  are  the  problems  of  human 
beings  themselves.  A  growing  sense  of  dissatisfaction  and  disillu- 
sionment with  things  as  they  are  today  is  causing  many  people  to 
question  traditional  conceptions  of  "human  nature'^wfficlt  have 
tended  to  be  taken  for  granted.  Many  social  scientists,  whos  per- 
haps feel  called  upon  even  more  than  psychologists  to  give  some 
explanation  or  to  find  some  solution  for  things  as  they  are,  almost 
inevitably  put  forth,  concoct,  or  rehash  verdicts  concerning  human 
problems  and  the  role  the  human  factor  plays  in  our  complex 
world.  Often  some  account  of  "human  nature"  is  vigorously  ad- 
vocated as  a  premise  with  which  to  justify  the  perpetuation  or 
acceptance  of  an  existing  set  of  human  relationships  in  religious, 
social,  or  economic  life. 

And  certain  aspects  of  these  accounts  of  "human  nature"  have 
important  bearings  on  ego  problems.  Some  people  argue  that 
human  beings  are  self-seeking.  Others  deny  it.  Some  maintain 
that  man  is  endowed  with  a  craving  for  power.  Some  find  pri- 
marily in  man's  nature  the  elemental  seeds  of  a  need  for  out- 
stripping others.  There  are  those  who  assume  that  "human 
nature"  is  the  source  of  all  harmony  and  solidarity.  Problems  of 
"human  nature,"  as  related  to  war  and  peace,  to  egotism  and  altru- 
ism, to  human  self-centeredness  or  selflessness,  are  among  the  cur- 
rent topics  of  hot  debate  among  a  good  many  people  within  and 
without  the  psychological  profession.  Such  issues  touch  closely  on 
the  facts  that  concern  us  in  this  book.  They  indicate  that  ego 
problems  are  matters  of  everyday  life  human  relationships;  that 
they  are  not  mere  academic  topics  to  be  discussed  and  argued  by 
those  who  can  afford  the  luxury  of  such  an  exclusive  pastime. 

The  word  "ego"  is  a  much-abused  word.  It  has  long  suffered 


2  INTRODUCTION 

scientifically  objectionable  associations  in  religious,  philosophical, 
ethical,  and  psychological  writings.  Because  of  this,  it  might  have 
been  safer  to  refer  to  the  mass  of  material  we  are  handling  in  this 
book  by  a  new  term.  But  psychology  today  seems  to  be  overflow- 
ing with  new  terms,  especially  among  those  who  write  on  topics 
concerning  social  psychology  and  who  create  concepts  to  suit  their 
personal  preferences.  So  we  reluctantly  chose  to  retain  the  word 
ego.  But  we  have  taken  pains  to  use  it  only  in  factually  demon- 
strable connections. 

For  there  is  great  need  for  such  a  concept.  The  differential 
character  of  a  tremendous  number  of  reactions  observed  both  in 
the  laboratory  and  in  everyday  life  must  be  designated  by  some 
such  concept.  The  ego  is  inferred  from  behavior  whether  verbal 
or  nonverbal  To  be  more  specific,  reactions  are  modified  and 
altered  to  a  greater  or  lesser  degree  when  they  are  ego-involved. 
The  ego,  as  we  shall  repeat  time  and  time  again,  is  not  an  entity. 
It  is  not  fixed  and  immutable  even  after  it  is  formed. 

In  the  following  chapters  we  have  preferred  to  use  as  much  as 
possible  terms  like  "ego-involved  reactions"  instead  of  "ego."  For 
expressions  such  as  "ego-involvements"  or  "ego-involved  reactions" 
are  less  tainted  with  the  objectionable  associations  which  "ego" 
has  suffered  historically  in  the  substantive  form.  The  expression 
"ego-involvements"  and  its  derivatives  have  come  into  use  in  psy- 
chology during  the  past  decade,  chiefly  in  connection  with  ex- 
perimental investigations  or  theories  related  to  empirical  evidence. 

An  innumerable  wealth  of  hitherto  disconnected  observations 
and  facts  have  all  converged  to  compel  us  to  formulate  certain 
conclusions  concerning  the  formation  and  functioning  of  a  con- 
stellation in  the  psychological  make-up  of  the  individual  which 
may  be  designated  as  the  ego.  There  are  observations  on  the 
genetic  development  of  the  integrated  person,  as  well  as  on  the 
varieties  of  disintegration  and  breakdown.  There  is  a  great  mass 
of  data  on  the  effects  of  collective  behavior.  There  is  an  impressive 
array  of  experimental  facts  obtained  during  the  last  ten  years  and 
concerned  with  the  differential  effects  of  ego-involvements. 

The  reason  these  scientifically  collected  data  lend  themselves  so 
readily  to  a  concise  formulation  is  that  they  have  their  concrete 


INTRODUCTION  3 

counterparts  in  everyday  life.  They  are  so  general  that  it  took 
almost  no  effort  on  our  part  to  find  further  verifying  evidence 
from  concrete  human  relationships  and  from  the  colorful  expres- 
sions of  personal  relationships  found  in  literary  works.  This  con- 
verging of  facts  from  so  many  different  sources  is,  in  a  nutshell, 
the  hypothesis  the  following  chapters  verify.  Dogmatic  statements 
presented  as  hypotheses  here  in  the  introduction  will  acquire  sub- 
stance or  remain  as  mere  idle  intellectual  chatter  on  the  basis  of 
the  evidence  we  have  tried  to  bring  together  in  this  volume. 

The  ego  is  not  innate  in  the  sense  that  certain  other  psychologi- 
cal functions,  such  as  perception,  are  innate.  Nor  is  ego-striving 
innate  in  the  sense  of  an  instinct,  drive,  or  need  such  as  the  innate 
striving  for  food,  water,  or  a  sex  object.  There  is  no  instinctive 
ego  drive,  no  innate  need  for  domination  or  submission,  for  suc- 
corance  or  idol  worship.  Nor  is  the  ego  a  basic  personality  pat- 
tern, unique  and  unchangeable  in  the  person.  That  constellation 
in  the  psychological  make-up  of  the  human  adult  which  may  be 
designated  as  the  ego  (or  by  any  other  convenient  concept)  is  a 
genetic  formation.  The  newborn  infant,  even  though  biologically 
integrated,  has  no  ego.  And  no  ego  would  develop  in  him 
through  maturation  if  it  were  not  for  two  facts.  First,  the  fact 
that  his  psychological  functioning — and  his  alone  in  the  whole 
animal  kingdom— can  take  place  on  a  conceptual  (symbolic)  level. 
This  enables  him  to  grasp  reciprocal  relationships  and  to  make 
effective  use  of  the  accumulation  of  diverse  concepts  and  symbols. 
Second,  once  man  is  equipped  biologically  with  the  possibility  of 
functioning  psychologically  on  a  conceptual  level,  he  has  to  live  in 
a  lawfully  ordered  world  of  nature  where  social  relationships  and 
their  products  impose  on  him  the  necessity  of  regulating  and 
adapting  himself  to  lawful  nature  and  to  the  established  order  of 
human  relationships.  Without  these  impositions,  these  restrictions, 
resistances,  and  rewards  of  nature  and  especially  of  the  established 
social  order  surrounding  him  with  its  material  and  technological 
products,  its  institutions,  its  accumulated  symbols,  values,  and 
norms,  there  would  be  no  consistent  and  continuous  ego  formation 
in  the  individual. 

This  is  substantiated  by  the  fact  that  the  egos  of  individual 
members  of  different  social  orders  are  shaped  in  their  major 


4  INTRODUCTION 

features  by  the  image  of  those  social  orders.  History  and  ethnol- 
ogy convincingly  show  that  these  .images  may  in  some  cases  be 
diametrically  opposite  in  character.  In  accounting  for  these  dia- 
metrically opposite  variations,  descriptions  of  the  ego  in  terms  of 
instincts  or  needs  with  long  and  impressive  lists  of  ego  drives 
utterly  fail  The  logical  argument  to  which  instinctive  ego  drive 
theories  must  resort  is  to  posit  the  existence  of  different  human 
species  at  different  times  and  in  different  places. 

In  brief,  the  ego  consists  of  many  attitudes  which  from  infancy 
on  are  related  to  the  delimited,  differentiated  and  accumulating 
"I,"  "me,"  "mine"  experiences.  These  attitudes,  which  may  be 
designated  as  ego-attitudes,  are  constituent  components  of  the  ego. 
Apart  from  the  constellation  of  these  ego-attitudes,  there  is  no  such 
entity  as  the  ego.  In  fact  many  attitudes  are  not  discrete  affairs  in 
the  psychological  make-up  of  individuals.  They  are  attitudes  that 
define  and  qualify  an  individual's  relative  standing  to  other  persons 
or  to  institutions  in  some  more  or  less  lasting  way.  They  are 
attitudes  that  determine  the  more  or  less  enduring  character  of 
one's  personal  identity  with  the  values  or  norms  incorporated  in 
him.  When  these  attitudes  are  situationally  called  for,  when  they 
are  at  any  time  consciously  or  unconsciously  involved  in  a  psy- 
chological function,  we  become  personally  involved.  And  when 
we  do  become  personally  involved,  then  our  discrimination,  judg- 
ment, perception,  remembering,  thinking,  and  explicit  behavior 
are  accordingly  modified  or  altered. 

Once  formed,  the  ego  is  by  no  means  rigid  and  unchangeable. 
To  a  large  extent,  ego-involvements  are  situationally  determined. 
The  same  words  of  insult  that  may  be  gracefully  swallowed  in  one 
situation  may  lead  to  violent  exhibitionistic  reactions  in  another. 
In  certain  cases  the  threshold  of  sensitivity  of  ego-involvements 
may  be  quite  low,  in  other  cases  quite  high,  depending  on  the 
demands  of  the  situation  and  the  psychological  condition  of  the 
individual.  These  thresholds  of  ego-involvement  may  be  measured 
in  an  empirical  way.  Since  attitudes,  the  constituents  of  the  ego, 
are  subject  to  change  and  transformation,  the  ego  is  also  subject  to 
chaqge  and  transformation  with  changing  situations  of  a  major 
character.  Such  transformation  can  be  clearly  observed  in  the 


INTRODUCTION  5 

changing  ego  of  the  adolescent  boy  or  girl,  especially  in  times  and 
in  societies  where  social  transition  is  rapid. 

The  psychology  of  the  formation  and  functioning  of  ego-atti- 
tudes is  governed  essentially  by  the  same  principles  as  the  general 
psychology  of  attitudes.  The  psychology  of  attitudes  is,  therefore, 
basic  to  the  psychology  of  the  ego.  Consequently,  we  have  had  to 
deal  first  and  at  some  length  with  the  psychology  of  attitudes  $nd 
carry  the  conclusions  reached  there  to  the  treatment  of  ego-in- 
volvements. Characterized  in  broad  outlines,  an  attitude  is  an 
established  readiness  which  has  a  subject-object  relationship  of 
highly  variable  content,  which  is  learned  (formed),  which  has 
affective  properties  with  various  degrees  of  motivational  compo- 
nents, which  may  refer  to  whatever  stimuli  are  encompassed  in 
the  subject-object  relationship,  and  which  determines  that  an  in- 
dividual will  react  to  a  stimulus  in  a  selective  way.  Once  formed, 
an  attitude  serves  as  an  anchorage  to  structure  or  modify  subse- 
quent experience  or  response. 

This  broad  characterization  of  attitudes  also  holds  basically  true 
in  the  case  of  ego-attitudes.  Many  attitudes  and  especially  ego- 
attitudes  are  formed  in  relation  to  and  directed  towards  objects  or 
persons  that  satisfy  basic  needs.  Hence  the  affective  and  emotional 
halo  of  many  ego-strivings  that  make  an  individual  highly  selec- 
tive and  highly  self-centered — sometimes  to  the  point  of  appearing 
deaf  and  blind  to  cues  in  objective  situations — spring  from  such 
relationships.  Many  of  the  attitudes  revealed  in  everyday  life 
contacts  define  an  individual's  status  for  him  in  relation  to  other 
people.  Many  embody  the  values  a  person  upholds  for  himself  and 
regulate  his  claims  and  aspirations  in  relation  to  others.  These 
ego-attitudes  are  therefore  highly  charged  affective  anchorages. 

Every  individual  strives  to  place  or  to  anchor  himself  as  an 
acceptable  member  in  his  social  milieu  or  in  some  social  setting, 
whatever  the  particular  criteria  for  acceptance  by  his  group  or  his 
aspired  group  may  be.  This  is  true  for  any  individual  in  any  cul- 
ture, whether  highly  competitive  or  highly  co-operative,  whether 
primitive  or  advanced.  There  is  an  unmistakable  striving  on  the 
part  of  the  individual  to  belong  to  his  group  or -to  some  aspired 
group.  Lack  of  social  belongingness  and  conflicts  in  belonging- 
ness  (marginality)  are  painful.  Once  the  ego  is  formed  to  the 


6  INTRODUCTION 

degree  of  grasping  reciprocal  human  relationships,  this  striving  to 
anchor  oneself  becomes  more  compelling  because  of  the  emotion- 
ally charged  character  of  ego-attitudes. 

Yet  mere  acceptance  of  an  individual  by  a  group  in  his  milieu 
or  by  some  group  he  aspires  to  will  not  do  forever.  The  mere 
sense  of  belongingness  does  not  bring  lasting  satisfaction.  A 
young  man  may  aspire,  as  an  ideal,  to  enter  a  certain  college.  But 
once  he  gets  there,  he  acquires  new  ideas  and  tries  to  be  somebody 
as  prescribed  by  the  hierarchical  arrangement  of  social  relation- 
ships in  the  college.  A  man  may  reach  his  ideal  of  belonging  to 
some  club.  But  after  admission  as  a  member,  he  may  set  his  eyes 
on  some  position  at  the  top  of  the  club's  social  pyramid.  In  groups 
everywhere  social  life  is  necessarily  hierarchical. 

The  striving  for  social  acceptance  and  approval,  which  at  times 
reaches  intense  proportions,  does  not  have  to  be  attributed  to  the 
urges  and  stresses  of  some  special  instinct.  All  attitudes  function 
in  well-formed  frames,  in  referential  settings.  If  the  situation  does 
not  produce  its  appropriate  frame  in  an  individual,  he  tends  con- 
sciously or  unconsciously  to  structure  the  situation,  at  least  pre- 
cariously, in  a  way  determined  by  the  stresses  of  his  internal  and 
internalized  desires,  deprivations,  and  anchorages.  Experiences  of 
ambiguity  and  confusion  are  tense  and  unpleasant.  Relief  from 
such  experiences  becomes  even  more  imperative  in  connection  with 
ego-attitudes,  for  these  delimit  and  define  one's  sense  of  personal 
security.  If  ego-attitudes  are  experienced  as  relatively  stable,  then, 
psychologically,  one's  sense  of  status  in  relation  to  other  persons, 
groups,  or  institutions  is  secure.  At  times,  any  status,  any  standing 
in  a  group  is  preferred  to  none,  even  though  it  may  be  felt  as  quite 
unsatisfactory  by  the  individual  himself.  Many  a  person  has  pre- 
ferred to  sweat  out  his  ego  problems  in  a  group  rather  than  face 
ostracism  or  break  away  into  uncertainty. 

It  seems  plausible  to  us  to  regard  individual  differences  of  ego- 
striving  as  due  to  individual  differences  in  glandular  activity  and 
other  temperamental  factors,  to  individual  differences  in  the  func- 
tioning of  basic  drives  such  as  those  for  food  and  sex,  and  to 
individual  differences  in  general  physical  vitality  and  intelligence. 
In  addition  to  such  basic  individual  differences,  of  course,  favor- 
able or  adverse  circumstancfes  for  the  satisfaction  of  basic  needs,  or 


INTRODUCTION  / 

for  the  attainment  of  a  secure  position,  further  contribute  to  differ- 
ential ego-strivings.  There  is  no  need  to  assume  any  differential 
ego  instinct. 

A  conservative  capitalist  and  a  revolutionary  communist  may 
both  defend  their  respective  views  with  equal  intensity.  A  Repub- 
lican and  a  Democrat  may  fight  one  another  tooth  and  nail  over 
the  Administration's  foreign  policy  and  may  both  be  frustrated 
persons.  The  millions  of  people  who  followed  Hitler  were  surely 
not  all  perverse  psychotic  creatures.  The  flourishing  post  mortem 
accounts  which  attribute  fascist  barbarism  in  Germany  to  the  per- 
verse complexes  of  Nazi  leaders  look  ridiculous  when  one  remem- 
bers the  predictions  and  warnings  made  to  the  world  in  all  earnest- 
ness in  the  early  1930's  by  those  observers  who  took  the  situational 
determination  of  social  movements  seriously. 

We  are  not  arguing  here  that  personality  characteristics,  indi- 
vidual frustrations,  or  elations  are  not  important  psychological 
facts.  Far  from  it.  We  have  to  learn  as  much  as  we  can  about 
them.  But  we  do  maintain,  on  the  basis  of  accumulating  evidence 
in  several  fields,  that,  in  dealing  with  the  identifications,  ego- 
involvements,  and  loyalties  of  individuals,  the  social  psychologist 
(for  these  are  social  psychological  problems)  should  start  first  by 
relating  the  individual  to  his  reference  and  membership  groups 
and  then  proceed  to  the  finer  details  of  personality  problems.  For 
an  individual's  identifications  and  ego-involvements,  his  more  or 
less  lasting  loyalties,  and  the  values  he  does  so  much  to  uphold  as 
his  own,  have  no  meaning  apart  from  his  reference  and  member- 
ship groups.  For  the  social  psychologist,  the  fact  that  a  person 
identifies  himself  with  the  Catholic  Church  or  with  a  revolution- 
ary party,  or  that  he  is  a  marginal  man  and  hence  has  conflicting 
loyalties,  is  at  least  as  important  as  a  knowledge  of  his  measurable 
or  unique  personality  traits  or  of  how  his  toilet  habits  were  handled 
in  infancy.  Once  a  person  is  placed  situationally  in  his  group 
setting,  then  information  concerning  his  personality  characteristics 
will  further  help  us  account  for  the<  differential  character  of  his 
reactions.  These  two  approaches  should  not  be  regarded  as  a 
dichotomous  antithesis  but  as  different  aspects  of  the  work  to  be 
done  if  the  picture  of  man's  ego-strivings  is  to  be  complete. 


8  INTRODUCTION 

In  this  volume  we  have  tried  to  give  only  the  broad  outlines  of 
the  psychology  of  ego-involvements.  It  is,  therefore,  an  incomplete 
treatment  of  the  subject.  The  all  important  topics  of  motivation 
and  personality  must  be  considered  in  further  work.  Although 
incomplete,  still,  in  our  opinion,  the  main  sketch  of  ego-involve- 
ments presented  here  will  not  have  to  be  changed  to  any  consider- 
able degree  when  motivational  and  personality  factors  are  brought 
into  the  picture.  For  the  values  one  upholds  as  his  personal  values, 
the  goals  he  sets  for  himself,  do  not  function  in  a  vacuum  or  in 
abstraction.  These  values  and  goals  as  well  as  all  major  ego-atti- 
tudes are  formed  in  or  in  relation  to  social  groups.  Investigations 
of  motivation,  personality  characteristics,  or  other  differential  qual- 
ities will  more  adequately  account  for  an  individual's  particular 
reactions  in  interpersonal  relationships  and  more  accurately  predict 
his  ego-involvements  in  a  given  situation  if  he  is  studied  first  in 
relation  to  his  reference  and  membership  groups.  It  seems  safe  to 
say  that  apart  from  a  person's  reference  of  himself  to  such  groups, 
he  would  not  develop  the  consistent  ego  which  defines  his  identity 
in  a  more  or  less  continuous  way  from  one  day  to  the  next. 


CHAPTER  2 
THE  PROBLEM  OF  ATTITUDES:  UNIFIED 

During  the  past  two  decades  the  problem  of  attitudes  has 
become  central  in  social  psychology.  Thus,  G.  W.  Allport  writes: 

The  concept  of  attitude  is  probably  the  most  distinctive  and  indis- 
pensable concept  in  contemporary  American  social  psychology.  No 
other  term  appears  more  frequently  in  experimental  and  theoretical 
literature.  [1,  798]  l 

Murphy,  Murphy,  and  Newcomb,  in  their  monumental  volume 
summarizing  the  state  of  social  psychology  in  1937,  again  empha- 
size the  point: 

Perhaps  no  single  concept  within  the  whole  realm  of  social  psy- 
chology occupies  a  more  nearly  central  position  than  that  of  attitudes. 
[19,  889] 

We  need  not  multiply  these  representative  statements  from  other 
sources  to  demonstrate  the  important  position  the  concept  of  atti- 
tudes holds  in  contemporary  social  psychology. 

The  study  of  attitudes  is  by  no  means  the  concern  of  psycholo- 
gists alone.  It  is  significant  that  sociologists  feel  as  much  at  home 
in  the  study  of  attitudes  as  do  psychologists.  Some  sociologists 
have  gone  so  far  as  to  equate  social  psychology  with  the  study  of 
attitudes.  [5,  11,  27]  Attitudes  became  a  focal  problem  in  experi- 
mental psychology  in  the  first  decade  of  this  century.  As  a  conse- 
quence of  the  introspective  analysis  of  the  higher  mental  processes 
at  Wiirzburg,  descriptions  were  given  in  "attitudinal"  terms  such 
as  Einstellung  and  Rewusstseinlagc.  [21, 30]  With  this  prominent 
start  in  experimental  psychology,  attitudes  came  to  stay  as  an  im- 
portant concept  in  that  field. 

In  this  book  we  are  proceeding  with  the  conviction  that  experi- 

1  Roman  numbers  inclosed  in  brackets  indicate  the  references  at  the  end  of  each 
chapter;  italic  numbers  indicate  page  numbers  in  those  references. 


10  THE  PROBLEM  OF  ATTITUDES:  UNIFIED 

mental  and  social  studies  on  attitudes  have  much  to  contribute  to 
each  other  in  bringing  about  a  unified  psychology  of  attitudes. 
The  psychologist's  task  is  to  give  an  adequate  account  of  the  psy- 
chological mechanisms  involved  in  the  formation  of  an  attitude  in 
any  individual.  The  formation  of  a  social  attitude  in  the  individ- 
ual should  be  essentially  the  same  as  the  formation  of  any  attitude 
if  the  explanation  of  this  formation  is  to  be  psychology  at  all. 
We  are  here  groping  in  this  direction.  As  we  shall  see,  attitude 
studies  at  present  do  not  give  us  a  unified  picture.  In  fact  the 
problem  of  attitudes  is  in  a  very  confused  state.  Perhaps  the  con- 
fusion has  increased  in  proportion  to  the  wealth  of  literature: 
different  investigators  have  had  different  interests  and  points  of 
view  in  approaching  the  problem. 

Attitudes  in  social  life.  When  we  look  at  any  society,  whether 
primitive  or  highly  developed,  whether  simple  or  complicated,  we 
observe  conformities  of  behavior,  within  the  limits  of  variations 
due  to  individual  differences,  on  the  part  of  the  individual  mem- 
bers of  any  society  as  they  carry  on  the  daily  business  of  living — 
for  example,  in  regulating  instinctive  activity,  dress,  likes  or  dis- 
likes of  other  groups,  or  responses  to  events  which  have  social 
significance.  When  analysis  and  explanation  of  these  established 
conformities  are  carried  to  the  psychological  level,  the  problem 
becomes  primarily  one  of  the  psychology  of  attitudes.2 

We  have  used  the  term  "established  conformity"  very  deliber- 
ately. For  these  established  conformities  result  from  conformity 
to  social  standards  or  norms  which  have  come  into  existence  in  a 
deterministic  way  as  a  consequence  of  the  interaction  of  individ- 
uals in  the  all  important  business  of  living.  Individual  members 
within  a  society  come  to  acquire  these  established  conformities  of 
experience  and  behavior  within  the  limits  of  their  individual 
differences.3  We  may,  in  fact,  say  in  a  summary  way  that  the 

2  The  question  of  whether  or  not  the  whole  of  social  psychology  consists  of  a 
psychology  of  attitudes  is  not  our  problem  here.  We  may  only  say  in  passing  that 
it  is  our  opinion  that  social  psychology  does  not  consist  only  of  the  psychology  of 
attitudes. 

8  Without  the  enormous  temperamental  differences  between  individuals  being 
in  any  way  ignored,  a  systematic  awareness  of  the  fact  that  temperamental  differ- 
ences always  function  within  a  given  social  context  and  set  of  norms  would  under- 
cut and  bring  into  proper  perspective  typologies  such  as  those  advanced  by  Jung, 
Spranger,  and  Sheldon,  which  attempt  to  explain  social  behavior  by  categorizing 
personality.  [15,  22,  25] 


ATTITUDE  STUDIES  AND  METHODOLOGICAL  CONSIDERATIONS        11 

socialization  which  occurs  when  an  individual  becomes  a  member 
of  a  group  consists  mainly  in  the  achievement  of  conformity  ia 
experience  and  behavior  to  social  values,  standards,  or  norms  al- 
ready established.  And  the  process  of  achieving  conformity  is,  if 
we  analyze  it  closely,  nothing  more  nor  less  than  the  formation 
of  appropriate  attitudes  in  relation  to  these  socially  standardized 
values  or  norms  or  other  criteria  of  conduct. 

RECENT  ATTITUDE  STUDIES  AND  METHODOLOGICAL  CONSIDERATIONS 

The  approach  to  the  psychology  of  attitude  has  been  many-sided. 
The  early  work  of  the  Wiirzburg  laboratory  gave  the  historical 
setting  to  more  recent  work  on  Aufgabe.  In  1918  Thomas  and 
Znaniecki  stimulated  other  sociologists  to  analyze  the  concept  of 
attitude  further  as  a  useful  tool  in  the  explanation  of  the  phenom- 
ena with  which  they  dealt.  The  work  of  cultural  anthropologists, 
such  as  Boas,  Malinowski,  and  Sapir,  and  of  many  contemporary 
ethnologists  highlights  the  variation  in  cultural  norms  and  the 
consequent  differences  in  the  attitudes  of  individuals  living  under 
different  social  systems. 

Investigators  such  as  G.  W.  and  F.  H.  Allport,  Daniel  Katz, 
Bain,  Paris  and  Lasker  concerned  themselves  with  the  genesis 
and  nature  of  attitudes.  F.  H.  Allport,  Thurstone,  Bogardus, 
Likert,  Droba,  and  others  worked  out  procedures  for  measuring 
attitudes.  The  attitudes  of  all  kinds  of  people  toward  almost  all 
conceivable  subjects  have  by  this  time  been  measured.  Quite  re- 
cently, the  interest  in  measuring  attitudes  has  given  way  to  the 
measurement  of  public  opinion  by  the  use  of  stratified  samples  of 
different  populations.  While  the  techniques  for  measuring  atti- 
tudes and  opinion  can  be  usefully  employed  to  gather  valuable 
information  on  attitude  determinants  and  properties,  and  while 
the  data  obtained  from  such  measurement  often  have  a  high  prac- 
tical, strategic,  or  systematic  value,  the  great  bulk  of  quantitative 
research  has  been  designed  for  the  primary  purpose  of  measure- 
ment alone,  not  systematic  understanding. 

Thomas  and  Znaniecki  held  that  "every  manifestation  of  con- 
scious life,  however  simple  or  complex,  general  or  particular,  can 
be  treated  as  an  attitude."  [27,  27]  In  a  critical  appraisal  of  their 
work,  Blumer  points  out  that  this  blanket  use  of  the  term  attitude 


12          "  THE  PROBLEM  OF  ATTITUDES:  UNIFIED 

makes  it  "a  kind  of  psychological  catchall,  since,  as  the  authors 
state,  it  may  refer  to  any  psychological  process,  or  item  of  con- 
sciousness. To  take  as  a  basic  datum  anything  that  includes  such 
diverse  things  as  appetites,  conceptions,  feelings,  decisions,  sensa- 
tions, desires,  ideas,  and  sentiments  is  to  operate  with  a  complicated 
and  indefinite  concept."  [4,  24  /.]  After  an  exhaustive  review  of 
the  literature  on  the  psychology  of  attitudes,  Nelson  reports  his 
impression,  shared  by  others,  "of  the  wide  variety  of  meanings 
which  are  ascribed  to  this  term."  [20]  He  cites  23  rather  distinct 
characterizations  given  the  term  "attitude"  by  psychologists  or 
social  scientists  up  to  1939,  ranging  from  "organic  drives,"  "neural 
sets,"  or  "trial  responses,"  to  "ways  of  conceiving  objects,"  or  sum 
totals  of  "inclinations,  feelings,  notions,  ideas,  fears,  prejudices, 
threats,  and  convictions  about  any  specific  topic."  In  a  recent 
paper  concerned  with  the  definition  and  use  of  "attitude"  in  social 
psychology,  Strauss  points  out  that  "the  concept,  despite  its  key 
position,  is  marked  by  considerable  confusion."  [26, 329]  He  notes 
that  much  of  the  research  on  attitudes  has  little  or  nothing  to  do 
with  attitude  theory  and  that  the  use  of  attitude  as  a  "common 
sense  explanation"  rather  than  as  a  "genuine  causal  explanation" 
[334]  has  retarded  systematic  understanding. 

It  is  therefore  not  surprising  that  some  strict  experimentalists, 
dissatisfied  with  what  they  may  regard  as  the  "practical"  or  un- 
systematic nature  of  attitude  research  in  social  psychology,  look 
down  on  it  and  on  social  psychology  in  general  as  having  little 
to  do  with  pure  science.  They  may  take  it  as  an  example  of  why 
the  pure  scientist  must  follow  Titchener's  dictum  and  stand  "apart 
from  the  great  majority  of  his  fellow  men,"  disavow  interpretation 
as  something  entirely  "foreign  to  him"  and  move  "in  the  domain 
of  bare  existence."  [28,  69  /.] 

The  laboratory  and  everyday  life.  Before  proceeding  with  a 
discussion  of  the  psychology  of  attitudes,  it  is  necessary  to  clear  up 
the  relationship  between  pure  experimental  research  and  the  sys- 
tematic study  of  attitudes  in  everyday  life  situations.  A  psycho- 
logical construct,  if  it  is  to  prove  valid  and  adequate,  must  be  as 
valid  and  adequate  in  handling  the  stuff  of  ordinary  human  affairs 
as  in  handling  the  controlled  variables  of  the  laboratory  experi- 
ment. Various  writers  have  fallen  into  the  error  of  making  a  dis- 


ATTITUDE  STUDIES  AND  METHODOLOGICAL  CONSIDERATIONS       13 

tinction  for  one  reason  or  another  between  "psychology"  and 
"social  psychology."  Klineberg,  for  example,  makes  such  a  dis- 
tinction "for  purposes  of  convenience"  [17,  4],  and  puts  his  em- 
phasis on  a  demonstration  of  "the  wealth  of  social  patterning" 
rather  than  on  the  "constants  of  human  behavior."  [8] 

Science  consists  of  a  set  of  conceptual  constructs  which  have 
high  predictive  value.  If  a  psychologist  is  to  make  any  claims  that 
will  have  scientific  validity,  he  must  obviously  be  as  objective  as 
possible.  This  does  not  mean,  however,  as  Titchener  believed, 
that  a  psychologist  must  be  "disinterested  and  impersonal."  The 
highbrow  superciliousness  of  those  who  hold  that  a  true  "scientific" 
psychologist  should  not  and  cannot  deal  with  the  concrete  realities 
of  men  in  their  social  life  comes  from  their  confusion  of  the  fact 
that  scientific  objectivity  derives  from  its  methods  and  not  from 
the  aloofness  of  its  observers.  [10]  William  James  long  ago 
pointed  out  that  psychology  is  scientific  to  the  extent  that  it  uses 
methods  which  make  verification  possible,  irrespective  of  the  the- 
ories, biases,  or  prejudices  of  the  experimenter.  "The  most  useful 
investigator,  because  the  most  sensitive  observer,"  said  James,  "is 
always  he  whose  eager  interest  in  one  side  of  the  question  is 
balanced  by  an  equally  keen  nervousness  lest  he  become  deceived." 
[14,  21] 

Since  the  scientist's  objectivity  derives  from  his  methods  rather 
than  from  his  own  interests  or  from  the  type  of  material  with 
which  he  deals,  and  since  the  psychologist's  goal  is  to  understand 
the  thought  and  behavior  of  men  in  real  life  situations,  the  scien- 
tific psychologist  becomes  obligated  to  formulate  concepts  that  will 
stand  up  both  in  the  laboratory  and  in  everyday  life.  [7]  His  task 
is,  of  course,  particularly  difficult  since  the  determinants  of  thought 
and  action  in  real  life  are  so  complicated,  the  context  within  which 
they  function  so  varied  and  changing. 

If  the  psychologist  becomes  fearful  of  the  consequences  of  cross- 
ing a  border  line  which  he  feels  separates  his  scientific  laboratory 
research  from  its  application  to  real  life  problems,  he  should  re- 
member that  the  concepts  of  chemistry,  physics,  and  biology  have 
been  developed  by  frequent  crossing  back  and  forth.  Theoretical 
concepts  arrived  at  in  the  laboratory  have  been  tested  for  their 
adequacy  in  normal  nonlaboratory  contexts.  In  the  history  of  the 


14  THE  PROBLEM  OF  ATTITUDES:  UNIFIED 

physical  sciences,  real  life  situations  have  forced  modifications  and 
revision  and  have  stimulated  further  research  which  has  produced 
more  adequate  constructs.  Julian  Huxley,  for  example,  has 
pointed  out  the  interdependence  of  application  and  theory  in  con- 
tributing to  the  advances  of  the  physical  sciences.  [13]  Giving 
examples  of  research  on  high  voltage  and  its  use  in  the  transmis- 
sion of  electricity,  he  notes  that  "sometimes  it  is  not  very  clear 
what  is  pure  and  what  is  applied,  or  in  which  direction  the  current 
is  flowing." 

Fortunately  for  psychology,  most  of  those  now  working  in  the 
field  are  becoming  increasingly  aware  of  the  challenge  which  the 
explanation  of  everyday  life  situations  poses  for  them.  We  mention 
the  problem  here  because  it  is  essential  to  our  argument  that  any 
final  conceptualization  of  the  nature  of  attitudes  must  make  quite 
explicit  the  mutual  dependence  of  data  obtained  from  the  labora- 
tory and  data  obtained  from  real  life  situations.  In  other  words, 
the  nature  of  attitudes  is  not  two  problems — one  for  die  laboratory 
and  one  for  everyday  life— but  one  single  problem. 

The  concepts  or  variables  thus  formulated  will  eventually  make 
it  possible  to  construct  a  psychology  of  attitudes  which  can  be  used 
to  study  any  kind  of  attitude,  social  or  nonsocial,  in  this  or  any 
other  social  system.  For  in  spite  of  cultural  diversities,  psycho- 
logical laws  ought  to  be  the  same  for  individuals  in  any  social 
system.  Otherwise,  logically  speaking,  we  should  be  arguing,  even 
if  we  do  not  mean  it,  for  some  sort  of  "cultural  racism,"  similar  to 
biological  racism.  If  psychological  laws  are  not  the  same  for  all 
individuals,  then  the  German  Kultur  apologists  were  right  in 
arguing  that  the  members  of  their  unique  Kultur  could  be  under- 
stood, not  explained,  only  by  die  peculiar  logic  of  their  Kultur.4 

Evaluation  of  norms.  We  must  make  one  more  prefatory  re- 
mark before  discussing  psychological  concepts  themselves:  explain 

4  In  the  field  of  anthropology  an  adequate  synthesis  of  the  psychology  of  attitude 
should  be  a  guide  to  more  adequate  generalizations  on  the  anthropologist's  own 
level.  Anthropological  concepts  which  try  to  fit  cultures  into  types  or  which  imply 
the  uniqueness  of  cultures  are  for  a  psychologist  quite  incomprehensible.  From 
our  point  of  view,  the  derivatives  of  different  economic  and  social  organizations 
studied  by  the  anthropologist  are  social  norms  and  values  which  act  as  social 
stimuli  and,  as  such,  have  characteristics  important  for  the  anthropologist  and  the 
social  psychologist  to  study. 


ATTITUDE  STUDIES  AND  METHODOLOGICAL  CONSIDERATIONS        15 

our  position  concerning  the  evaluation  of  standards,  social  values, 
or  norms,  since  this  position  has  important  ideological  implica- 
tions. We  repeat  that  standards,  social  values,  and  norms  are  the 
product  of  human  interaction  in  the  process  of  living.  There  is 
no  finality  about  them.  They  are  not  absolute.  In  spite  of  their 
inertia  after  they  once  come  into  existence,  and  in  spite  of  the 
efforts  of  those  who  for  one  reason  or  another  are  vitally  interested 
in  preserving  the  established  order,  we  observe  in  the  course  of 
history  that  social  standards,  values,  or  norms  do  change  as  a  con- 
sequence of  new  modes  of  human  interaction  brought  about  by 
changes  in  technological  and  economic  conditions. 

Any  given  individual  is  confronted  with  the  social  standards, 
values,  or  norms  of  his  environment.  In  short,  social  norms  are 
first  external  to  the  individual  or  on  the  stimulus  side  with  respect 
to  him.  As  such,  social  standards,  values,  or  norms  are  first  of  all 
the  data  of  the  social  sciences,  not  of  psychology.  Strictly  speak- 
ing, then,  the  task  of  the  psychologist  is  to  study  the  formation  of 
attitudes  as  a  consequence  of  contact  with  these  social  norms 
through  other  individuals  or  groups  or  through  various  products 
of  the  social  and  economic  environment.  In  spite  of  the  diversity 
and  variations  of  social  standards,  values,  or  norms  in  different 
societies,  human  beings  do  by  and  large  form  attitudes  in  con- 
formity to  their  group. 

The  psychologist  and  the  social  scientist,  however,  cannot  com- 
placently stop  at  this  point.  They  can  go  further  and  show  the 
consequences  of  the  experience  and  behavior  which  are  regulated 
by  attitudes  developed  and  prescribed  by  existing  social  norms. 
On  the  basis  of  such  studies,  they  can  reach  conclusions  as  to 
whether  or  not  the  attitudes  formed  are  conducive  to  a  harmonious 
and  well-adjusted  personality  or  to  a  contradictory  and  unadjusted 
personality,  whether  or  not  the  attitudes  formed  bring  about  social 
solidarity  or  friction.  In  short,  the  psychologist  and  the  social 
scientist  can  reach  conclusions  concerning  the  extent  to  which 
attitudes  formed  from  prescribed  social  norms  conform  to  or  are 
contradictory  to  the  objective  conditions  existing  at  any  given 
period  in  a  given  social  system. 

Our  present  tas\.  We  have  seen  that  in  spite  of  the  wealth  of 
material  in  the  field  of  attitudes,  we  still  do  not  have  a  unified  and 


16  THE  PROBLEM  OF  ATTITUDES:  UNIFIED 

established  psychology  of  attitudes.  The  phenomena  covered  by 
the  concept  "attitude"  vary  to  a  large  degree  in  their  specificity  and 
range.  On  the  one  hand  we  have  strict  experimental  laboratory 
investigators  interested  in  the  attitude  or  set  to  a  very  limited  and 
precise  stimulus  situation  in  a  laboratory  setup;  on  the  other  hand 
we  find  social  psychologists  and  social  scientists  working  on  atti- 
tudes of  individuals  or  groups  to  a  whole  nation  or  race,  or  to 
values  and  concepts  which  have  wide  extensions  such  as  the  con- 
cepts of  fascism  and  democracy.  It  is  no  wonder,  therefore,  that 
investigators  working  on  one  level  of  generality  often  use  concepts 
which  have  very  little  meaning  or  relevance  for  investigators 
working  on  a  different  level  of  generality. 

It  is  our  methodological  conviction  that  attitudes  in  strict  labora- 
tory situations  and  attitudes  in  the  most  complicated  social  situation 
have,  essentially,  the  same  psychological  mechanism  at  bottom, 
that  the  basic  psychological  substrata  functioning  in  both  instances 
are  the  same  in  nature.  As  a  result  of  brilliant  experiments  still  in 
progress  on  the  relation  of  vision  to  human  behavior,  Adelbert 
Ames  of  the  Dartmouth  Eye  Institute  has  concluded  in  a  prelimi- 
nary report  that  "The  processes  that  underlie  our  perception  of  our 
immediate  external  world  and  those  that  underlie  our  perception 
of  social  relationships  are  fundamentally  the  same.  The  insights 
gained  in  the  study  of  visual  sensation  can  serve  as  indispensable 
leads  to  better  understanding  and  more  effective  handling  of  the 
complexities  of  social  relationships."  [2,  7]  This  is,  of  course,  not 
in  the  least  a  denial  of  the  rich  and  complicated  motivational, 
affective,  and  cognitive  factors  involved  in  social  or  interpersonal 
attitudes.  But,  in  order  to  develop  concepts  which  have  scientific 
generality  of  the  kind  found  in  the  natural  sciences,  we  must  give 
up  the  purely  descriptive  concepts  used  by  social  psychologists  and 
sociologists  and  see  what  functional  variables  have  been  well  estab- 
lished and  verified  in  experimental  psychology  and  other  con- 
trolled investigation.  Once  equipped  with  these  well-established 
variables  or  concepts,  we  can  then  proceed  to  see  if  we  can  extend 
these  to  the  more  complicated  cases  of  attitudes  which  lead  the 
individual  to  react  in  characteristic  ways  in  actual  social  inter- 
action. 

Our  specific  task  then  becomes  self-evident.  We  shall  look  for 


A  CHARACTERIZATION  OF  ATTITUDES  17 

the  most  essential  criteria  which  can  be  detected  in  any  attitude— 
from  the  relatively  simple  case  of  the  laboratory  setup  to  the  more 
complicated  situations  in  actual  social  life.  We  shall  proceed  with 
a  minimum  of  assumptions, 

A  CHARACTERIZATION  OF  ATTITUDES  8 

Attitudes  are  among  those  components  of  the  psychological 
make-up  of  the  individual  which  determine  that  he  shall  react, 
not  in  a  passive  or  neutral  way,  but  in  a  selective  and  characteristic 
way,  especially  in  relation  to  certain  specific  stimulus  situations. 
Attitudes  are  not,  of  course,  the  only  psychological  components 
or  states  that  determine  that  an  individual  will  react  to  the  envi- 
ronment in  a  selective  or  characteristic  way.  When  the  individual 
is  hungry,  thirsty,  or  sexually  aroused,  or  in  some  other  emotional 
state,  or  has  been  recently  stimulated  by  a  functional  change  in 
the  receptor  organ  or  in  the  organism  at  large,  he  reacts  in  selec- 
tive or  characteristic  ways  to  the  environment.  Attitudes,  then, 
are  among  the  various  psychological  factors  which  determine  the 
individual's  selective  reaction  to  his  environment. 

In  all  the  representative  definitions  or  characterizations  of  atti- 
tudes, one  feature  is  common:  that  an  attitude,  whatever  else  it 
may  be,  denotes  a  functional  state  of  readiness  which  determines 
the  organism  to  react  in  a  characteristic  way  to  certain  stimuli  or 
stimulus  situations.  A  glance  at  some  representative  definitions 
of  attitudes,  imposes  on  us  the  fact  that  their  essential  feature  is  a 
functional  state  of  readiness. 

The  "attitude"  is  primarily  a  way  of  being  "set"  toward  or  against 
certain  things.  [Murphy  and  Murphy,  18] 

[Attitude]  is  readiness  for  attention,  or  action,  of  a  definite  sort. 
[Baldwin,  3] 

Attitude — the  specific  mental  disposition  toward  an  incoming  (or 
arising)  experience,  whereby  that  experience  is  modified,  or,  a  condi- 
tion of  readiness  for  a  certain  type  of  activity;  .  .  .  [Dictionary  of 
Psychology,  Warren,  29] 

An  attitude  is  a  more  or  less  permanently  enduring  state  of  readiness 
of  mental  organization  which  predisposes  an  individual  to  react  in  a 

6  This  characterization  follows  that  presented  by  Sherif  in  [23]  and  [24]. 


18  THE  PROBLEM  OF  ATTITUDES:  UNIFIED 

characteristic  way  to  any  object  or  situation  with  which  it  is  related, 
[Cantril,  6] 

After  reviewing  these  and  other  representative  characterizations 
of  attitudes,  G.  W.  Allport  reaches  the  conclusion  that  "the  essen- 
tial feature  of  attitude"  is  "a  preparation  or  readiness  for  response." 
Allport  gives  his  own  definition  in  which  this  functional  state  of 
readiness  is  essential: 

An  attitude  is  a  mental  and  neural  state  of  readiness,  organized 
through  experience,  exerting  a  directive  or  dynamic  influence  upon 
the  individual's  response  to  all  objects  and  situations  with  which  it  is 
related.  [1] 

Whatever  other  features  attitudes  may  have  (and  the  features  of 
attitudes  do  vary  according  to  the  degree  of  complexity),  it  is 
certain  that  all  attitudes  have  a  state  of  readiness  in  common. 
However,  every  state  of  readiness  of  the  organism  is  not  an  atti- 
tude. There  are  numerous  states  of  readiness  which  cannot  be 
called  attitudes.  For  example,  a  child  of  two  or  three  is  hardly 
ever  neutral  or  passive  to  his  environment.  He  is  apt  to  be  ex- 
tremely partial  one  way  or  another  to  the  things  or  persons  sur- 
rounding him.  He  is  constantly  seeking  all  sorts  of  satisfaction. 
All  these  momentary  tendencies  imply  states  of  readiness.  But  in 
spite  of  all  this,  we  can  hardly  say  that  a  child  of  three  is  full  of 
attitudes.  In  fact  he  has  very  few  (if  any)  established  or  stable 
attitudes.  He  may  have  developed  some  attitudes  toward  certain 
persons  like  his  mother  or  certain  objects  such  as  types  of  food. 
But  a  child  at  this  age  will  even  react  in  a  very  negative  way  to 
his  mother  if  she  proves  to  be  an  obstacle  in  his  effort  to  gain  some 
satisfaction  at  the  moment.  We  need  not  elaborate  here  other 
cases  of  readiness  which  cannot  properly  be  labeled  as  attitudes. 

We  must  conclude  therefore  that  cases  of  readiness  are  not 
exhausted  by  all  cases  of  attitudes.  The  state  of  readiness  of  the 
organism  is  a  more  general  term.  Attitudes  constitute  special  cases 
of  readiness.  Therefore,  we  must  have  some  concrete  criteria 
which  single  out  cases  of  readiness  as  attitudes.  It  seems  to  us 
that  the  following  five  criteria  are  found  in  cases  of  readiness 
which  are  labeled  as  attitudes. 


A  CHARACTERIZATION  OF  ATTITUDES  19 

1.  Attitudes  always  imply  a  subject-object  relationship.    Atti- 
tudes are  always  related  to  definite  stimuli  or  stimulus  situations. 
These  may  be  objects  stich  as  home,  automobile,  souvenir,  some 
particular  eating  place;  persons  such  as  one's  own  body,  mother, 
father,  brother,  some  friend,  rival,  teacher,  sweetheart,  wife;  or 
groups  of  people  such  as  classmates,  playmates,  Negroes,  the  com- 
munity; institutions  such  as  the  school,  college,  church,  club;  or 
socially  established  and  standardized  concepts,  values  or  norms 
such  as  the  flag,  the  Constitution,  democracy.   These  subject-object 
relationships  are  not  innate,  are  not  biologically  given.    The  items 
or  objects  toward  which  the  subject-object  relationship  is  devel- 
oped are  always  first  on  the  stimulus  side  for  the  individual.    Only 
after  contact  with  these  outside  stimuli,  does  any  relationship 
develop  between  them  and  the  individual. 

In  various  definitions  of  attitude,  the  content  of  an  attitude  is 
often  mentioned  in  such  terms  as  "objects,"  "social  stimuli,"  "social 
objects."  [20]  But  the  content  of  attitudes  will  depend  upon  the 
particular  nature  of  the  subject-object  relationship  established. 
The  contents  of  attitudes  are  as  numerous  and  as  different  as  the 
stimulus  situations  to  which  attitudes  are  related. 

2.  This  means  that  attitudes  are  formed  and  formed  in  relation 
to  objects,  persons,  and  values  which  may  or  may  not  have  moti- 
vational appeal  at  first.    Almost  any  food  may  satisfy  hunger,  but 
we  may  develop  a  special  liking  for  a  special  food,  even  for  a 
special  restaurant,  and  even  a  special  table  in  that  restaurant. 
When  these  particular  likes  or  dislikes  are  more  or  less  fixated, 
we  have  formed  attitudes  in  relation  to  these  particular  objects. 
Almost  any  average  member  of  the  opposite  sex  will  satisfy  sexual 
need,  but  when  it  is  fixated,  with  all  its  affective  overtones,  it 
becomes   an  attitude— an  attitude  toward   a  particular  person. 
George  Bernard  Shaw  has  aptly  defined  a  lover  as  a  man  who 
exaggerates  the  difference  between  one  woman  and  another. 

Since  attitudes  are  not  innate  states  of  readiness,  inasmuch  as 
they  are  formed  in  relation  to  particular  objects,  persons,  institu- 
tions, and  values  or  norms,  the  individual  has  first  to  come  into 
contact  with  them.  And  coming  into  contact  is  a  perceptual  situa- 
tion. This  means  that  the  primary  stage  in  the  formation  of  atti- 
tudes is  a  perceptual  stage.  The  presence  of  a  perceptual  stage  is 


20  THE  PROBLEM  OF  ATTITUDES:  UNIFIED 

of  the  utmost  psychological  importance.  For  certain  basic  facts 
about  perceptual  situations  provide  the  starting  point  for  the 
formation  of  attitudes,  as  we  shall  see  in  the  next  chapter. 

The  perceptual  stage  in  the  formation  of  attitudes  is  especially 
important  in  cases  of  attitudes  which  do  not  have  a  motivational 
(instinctual)  basis.6  As  the  accumulating  investigations  in  the 
field  of  attitudes  show,  many  social  attitudes  are  formed  through 
verbal  judgments  of  adults.  Indeed  the  most  directive  and  impor- 
tant social  attitudes  which  determine  status,  social  distance,  and 
the  like  seem  to  be  formed  through  verbalized  short-cut  dictums 
or  value  judgments  and  through  situations  which  outwardly  do 
not  have  any  momentary  motivational  appearance. 

The  fact  that  attitudes  are  not  innate  but  are  formed  as  a  result 
of  the  individual's  contact  with  his  environment  means,  of  course, 
that  attitudes  are  learned  or  conditioned.  Just  what  the  psycho- 
logical or  physiological  mechanisms  of  this  learning  may  be  are 
irrelevant  to  the  present  discussion.  We  are  concerned  here  with 
demonstrable  psychological  properties  and  characteristics  of  atti- 
tudes. Obviously,  the  more  adequate  the  psychology  of  learning 
or  conditioning  becomes,  the  better  will  we  understand  the  proc- 
esses involved  in  attitude  formation.  But  it  is  almost  inconceivable 
that  any  final  adequate  account  of  learning  developed  in  the  future 
would  ever  negate  the  statement  that  attitudes  are  formed. 

3.  Attitudes  have  affective  properties  of  varying  degrees.  Estab- 
lished attitudes  are  charged  with  affective  or  value  properties  in 
varying  degrees.  The  affective  property  of  attitudes  may  be  due 
to  motivational  (instinctual)  origins  such  as  hunger  and  sex  (as 
exemplified  in  cases  of  attitudes  toward  a  certain  food,  a  certain 
restaurant,  a  sweetheart  or  wife)  or  may  be  due  to  nonmotivational 
sources  (noninstinctual). 

The  affective  property  of  attitudes  with  instinctual  origins  is 
self-explanatory.  The  affective  property  of  attitudes  with  non- 
instinctual  sources  is  due  to  the  fact  that  these  attitudes  are  formed 
in  relation  to  social  values  or  norms  which  in  themselves  are 
standardized  affective  fixations.  They  are  usually  verbalized  short- 
cut value  judgments  such  as  "the  home  is  a  sacred  institution." 

6  We  are  using  the  term  "motivation"  here  to  denote  only  drives,  needs,  or 
instincts,  which  have  a  definite  origin  in  the  organism  of  the  individual. 


A  CHARACTERIZATION  OF  ATTITUDES  21 

Value  judgments  are  always  given  in  adjectival  form.  And  all 
judgments  given  in  terms  of  adjectives  certainly  have  affective 
properties.  The  social  values  presented  in  short-cut  dictums,  or  in 
other  ways  accompanied  by  praise  or  blame,  naturally  are  affec- 
tively charged.  The  individual  is  forced  to  respect  and  uphold  the 
values  of  the  family,  school,  church,  or  other  institutions  he  is  a 
member  or  would-be  member  of.  If  he  does  not  respect  and  glory 
in  his  flag,  he  is  compelled  to.  The  very  fact  of  membership  and 
participation  in  group  activity  or  ceremony  makes  certain  stand- 
ardized values  or  practices  sacred,  justifiable,  right,  honorable,  or 
dutiful  in  the  individual's  eyes.  Consequently,  the  attitudes  an 
individual  forms  in  relation  to  such  activities  or  practices  become 
affectively  charged. 

Another  important  reason  why  attitudes  are  affectively  charged 
is  the  fact  that  many  attitudes  prescribe  the  individual's  relation- 
ship, status,  or  role  with  respect  to  other  individuals  or  groups 
(such  as  teacher,  worker,  boss,  minister,  assistant).  And  experi- 
ences connected  with  status  are  affectively  charged.7 

4.  Attitudes  are  more  or  less  enduring  states  of  readiness. 
There  are  states  of  readiness  which  are  more  or  less  momentary, 
depending  on  the  state  of  the  organism  and  the  situation  at  the 
time.  For  example,  we  may  be  very  hungry  and  snatch  a  loaf  of 
bread.  After  we  have  eaten  enough  and  become  satisfied,  the  loaf 
may  be  pushed  aside.  At  the  time  of  sexual  tension,  a  person 
toward  whom  there  is  no  established  attitude  but  who  can  satisfy 
the  sexual  need  may  be  passionately  seized,  but,  after  the  need  is 
satisfied  so  that  the  tension  is  resolved,  one  may  never  look  at  the 
person  again.  In  these  cases  the  state  of  readiness  dissolves  as  the 
satiation  point  is  reached,  at  least  for  the  time  being.  But  not  so 
with  attitudes.  They  are  more  or  less  enduring  states  of  readiness. 
Thus  a  wounded  soldier  tries  to  show  his  respect  to  his  superior 
officer  who  has  really  shared  the  hardships  with  him.  A  very 
much  preferred  food  may  be  the  subject  of  praise  after  the  point  of 
satiation  has  been  reached.  A  sweetheart  still  has  the  sentimental 
halo  in  the  eyes  of  her  lover  even  after  sexual  satisfaction.  A  dear 

7  Without  making  a  special  issue  of  it  at  this  point,  we  may  note  in  passing 
that  attitudes  related  to  role  or  status  are  ego-involved.  And  ego-involved  experi- 
ences and  responses  have  affective  properties  (sec  chs.  5,  6,  11). 


22  THE  PROBLEM  OF  ATTITUDES:  UNIFIED 

friend  is  still  liked  even  during  moments  of  minor  friction.  Some 
people  in  India  actually  starve  to  death  rather  than  eat  meat  which 
is  "forbidden"  by  the  very  rigid  norms  established  and  accepted  by 
Hindus.  These  examples  are  sufficient  to  illustrate  the  fact  that 
attitudes,  once  formed,  are  more  or  less  enduring  states  of  readi- 
ness, quite  independent,  within  limits,  of  the  momentary  states  of 
the  organism. 

An  attitude  becomes  a  more  or  less  enduring  state  of  readiness 
because  of  the  cognitive  components  in  its  formation.  We  have 
seen  that  attitudes  are  not  innate  entities,  that  they  are  formed  as 
a  consequence  of  contact  with  objects,  persons,  or  situations  to 
which  they  are  related.  We  referred  to  these  contacts  as  the  per- 
ceptual stage  in  the  formation  of  an  attitude.  It  is  this  perceptual 
stage  which  begins  to  give  an  attitude  its  cognitive  component  in 
the  process  of  formation.  Since  the  first  stage  in  the  formation  of 
an  attitude  is  a  perceptual  stage,  we  can  begin  at  once  to  utilize 
the  concepts  developed  to  account  for  perceptual  situations.  Here 
we  are  on  relatively  safe  ground.  The  fact  that  attitudes  are  more 
or  less  enduring  indicates,  further,  that  attitudes  are  learned. 
And  the  more  adequate  the  account  of  learning  becomes,  the  bet- 
ter will  we  be  able  to  understand  the  basis  for  an  attitude's  en- 
during quality,  just  as  we  will  better  understand  the  basis  of  its 
formation.  And  it  is  also  almost  inconceivable  that  any  final  ex- 
planation of  learning  would  alter  the  statement  that  attitudes  are 
more  or  less  enduring. 

We  should  be  very  clear  on  one  point,  however,  so  as  to  avoid 
misunderstanding.  We  have  said  that  attitudes  are  more  or  less 
enduring  states  of  readiness  to  stimuli,  objects,  persons,  groups, 
values,  or  norms,  in  relation  to  which  they  are  formed  and  which 
determine  the  individual  to  react  in  a  characteristic  way  in  relation 
to  them.  But  attitudes  are  not  absolute  fixed  states  of  readiness. 
Since  they  are  formed  as  a  consequence  of  contact  with  objects, 
values,  or  norms  to  which  they  are  related,  they  may  change  or 
disintegrate.  For  example,  good  friends  may  become  deadly  ene- 
mies; a  religious  person  may  become  an  atheist;  a  conservative 
may  become  radical  as  a  result  of  contact  with  new  facts  and 
events. 


A  CHARACTERIZATION  OF  ATTITUDES  23 

5.  Attitudes  range  in  the  number  and  variety  of  stimuli  to  which 
they  are  referred.  Since  attitudes  develop  as  a  consequence  of 
experience,  and,  since  attitudes  involve  a  cognitive  component,  the 
extent  or  range  of  stimuli  to  which  an  individual  will  relate  an 
attitude  will  vary  according  to  the  nature  of  the  source  of  the 
attitude  and  according  to  the  relationship  the  individual  makes 
between  an  attitude  and  the  stimulus  situation  he  confronts. 
Although  certain  attitudes,  especially  some  of  those  created  in  lab- 
oratory situations  or  those  of  children,  may  only  be  evoked  by  the 
situation  under  which  they  originally  developed,  the  more  usual 
process  is  that  an  attitude,  once  established,  will  be  related  by  the 
individual  to  a  variety  of  objects  or  situations  that  have  not  neces- 
sarily been  active  in  its  original  establishment. 

The  wide  range  exhibited  by  some  attitudes  is  possible  because 
of  the  fact  that  the  stimulus  situation  toward  which  the  attitude 
has  developed  is  itself  extensive,  that  is  to  say,  possible  of  repre- 
sentation or  expression  in  many  different  specific  contexts.  When 
we  noted  as  our  first  characteristic  of  attitude  that  it  always  im- 
plied a  subject-object  relationship,  we  pointed  out  that  we  could 
label  institutions,  concepts,  social  values,  or  norms  as  "objects"  in 
this  sense.  Social  attitudes  toward  such  stimulus  situations  are, 
we  have  said,  often  derived  from  the  verbal  judgments  of  others. 
For  example,  once  an  individual  has  accepted  the  value  judgment 
of  his  group  that  "Negroes  are  inferior"  and  should  occupy  a 
lower  status  in  society,  he  can  and  does  easily  relate  his  acquired 
attitude  to  innumerable  specific  situations.  As  we  shall  see,  a 
considerable  body  of  research  in  the  past  two  decades  has  shown 
that  attitudes  have  a  directive  effect  in  specific  situations,  that  a 
very  general  attitude  will  reveal  itself  in  a  wide  variety  of  ways.8 

Classifications  of  attitudes.  This  approach  to  the  problem  of 
attitudes  renders  meaningless  various  attempts  to  classify  them. 
G.  W.  Allport  has  summarized  the  major  varieties  of  classification: 

8  In  this  connection,  the  experimental  work  on  meaning  and  association  time  is 
significant,  in  view  of  the  fact  that  the  upshot  of  the  controversy  was  that  meaning 
is  more  immediate  than  specific  association.  (See  the  experiments  of  Moore, 
Tolman,  Weld,  and  Cantril.)  This  being  the  case,  the  immediate  "meaning'* 
reaction  determines  the  range  rather  than  specific  associations.  This  is  illustrated  in 
the  references,  for  example,  to  Kay's  study  of  personal  and  general  frames  [16] 
and  Horowitz's  study  on  the  development  of  race  attitudes.  [12] 


24  THE  PROBLEM  OF  ATTITUDES:  UNIFIED 

positive  and  negative,  specific  and  general,  public  and  private, 
common  and  individual  attitudes.  [1]  Since  the  characteristics  of 
any  attitudes  in  any  individual  will  vary  according  to  the  situation 
or  circumstances  under  which  the  attitude  has  developed  and  the 
function  the  attitude  serves  for  the  individual,  any  classification  of 
attitudes  becomes  almost  as  nebulous  as  any  classification  of  stimu- 
lus situations  (including  objects,  persons,  groups,  values,  and 
norms)  or  of  personal  and  societal  relationships.  Simple  dichoto- 
mous  classifications  especially  distort  and  falsify  the  problem. 
Take,  for  example,  the  distinction  made  by  Thomas  and  Znaniecki, 
between  "common"  and  "individual"  attitudes.  Their  use  of  the 
word  "common"  refers  to  attitudes  "common  to  all  conscious 
beings,"  their  use  of  "individual"  refers  to  attitudes  "peculiar  to 
only  one  individual  member  of  the  group."  [27,  18]  Such  a  dis- 
tinction makes  no  place  for  the  attitudes  of  members  of  small  or 
large  groups,  or  the  attitudes  characteristic  of  a  given  social  sys- 
tem, not  to  mention  the  elaborate  and  tenuous  classification  that 
would  be  necessary  to  place  an  attitude  according  to  the  subtle 
variations  it  might  have  for  the  individual  with  reference  to  the 
attitudes  of  other  members  of  the  same  group  or  social  system. 
And  just  because  some  attitudes  are  common  to  a  large  enough 
number  of  people  to  lend  themselves  to  measurement  does  not 
mean  that  a  distinction  between  "common"  and  "individual" 
attitudes  has  any  more  than  a  loose  descriptive  value.  Classifica- 
tions of  attitudes,  like  classifications  of  personalities,  represent  a 
rigid  structural  approach  which  is  apt  to  obscure  or  be  mistaken 
for  a  truly  functional  analysis. 

The  psychology  of  value  included  in  the  psychology  of  attitudes. 
We  have  said  previously  that  attitudes  always  imply  a  subject- 
object  relationship  and  that  attitudes  are  affectively  charged. 
Hence  the  stimulus  or  object  or  group  to  which  an  attitude  is 
related  is  reacted  to  affectively.  In  other  words,  the  stimulus, 
object,  person,  group,  or  norm  in  relation  to  which  an  attitude  is 
formed  has  value,  either  positive  or  negative  and  in  different 
degrees  for  that  individual.  We  are  using  the  word  "value"  to 
denote  these  affective  qualities.  Therefore,  the  problem  of  value 
as  an  affective  quality  is  part  of  the  psychology  of  attitude. 


A  CHARACTERIZATION  OF  ATTITUDES  25 

We  do  not  mean  to  say  that  all  attitudes  are  social  attitudes  or 
that  all  attitudes  are  related  to  social  values.  An  attitude  may  be 
developed  toward  certain  woods  in  which  a  person  has  taken  soli- 
tary walks,  or  an  attitude  may  be  formed  in  relation  to  almost 
anything  peculiar  to  the  surroundings  and  experiences  of  the 
individual.  And  we  might  mention  in  passing  the  abnormal 
fixations  sometimes  made  by  individuals  on  queer  and  unusual 
objects. 

In  spite  of  this  fact,  however,  the  attitudes  most  important  in 
daily  life  are  social  attitudes — attitudes  formed  in  relation  to  other 
individuals,  groups,  institutions,  tools  and  technology,  standardized 
values,  or  norms.  These  are  the  attitudes  that  really  determine  an 
individual's  reaction  to  other  people,  other  groups,  and  that  map 
out  for  him  the  main  boundaries  of  his  experience  and  taste. 
Most  of  these  social  attitudes  are  transmitted  by  short-cut  verbal 
value  judgments.  Words  are  the  most  common  medium  for  both 
the  formation  and  the  expression  of  social  attitudes.  And  it  is 
probably  for  this  reason  that  some  psychologists  have  character- 
ized attitudes  as  verbalized  dispositions.  Although  we  know  the 
social  attitudes  of  others  largely  from  the  words  they  use,  most  of 
us  do  have  many  quite  personal  attitudes  not  related  to  social 
values,  which  we  may  seldom  if  ever  express — attitudes  toward 
some  loved  one,  some  one  we  strongly  dislike,  some  personal 
keepsake,  some  house.  To  be  sure,  if  we  are  probed  concerning 
these  things,  we  will  be  able  to  express  our  attitude.  But  many 
attitudes  we  may  have  which  we  may  never  verbalize  are  just  as 
much  "attitudes"  as  those  we  do  verbalize  spontaneously.  The 
point  is  that  all  attitudes,  whether  social  or  nonsocial,  whether 
verbalized  or  nonverbalized,  function  essentially  according  to  the 
same  psychological  principles,  even  though  there  may  be  differ- 
ences of  content,  richness,  compellingness,  or  endurance. 

Although  the  psychology  of  values  is  involved  in  the  psychology 
of  attitudes,  it  should  be  borne  in  mind  that  social  values,  being 
first  on  the  stimulus  side  in  relation  to  the  individual,  are  essen- 
tially the  data  of  the  social  sciences.  Social  psychologists  become 
interested  in  and  concerned  with  social  values  simply  because 
social  values  are  part,  and  an  important  part,  of  the  stimuli  that 
surround  man  and,  through  stimulation,  influence  him. 


26  THE  PROBLEM  OF  ATTITUDES:  UNIFIED 

Special  cases  of  attitudes.  Various  terms  such  as  "set,"  "stereo- 
type," "prejudice,"  and  "opinion"  may  all  be  regarded  as  attitudes 
with  particular  characteristics  which  have  been  given  certain  labels 
by  common  use.  The  psychological  states  described  by  all  of  these 
terms  are  developed  in  relation  to  certain  stimuli  in  the  identical 
way  that  attitudes  are  developed.  All  are  affectively  charged  in 
relation  to  the  stimuli.  All  are  more  or  less  lasting,  all  are  ac- 
quired states  of  readiness  determining  the  individual's  character- 
istic reactions  to  the  stimuli  to  which  they  are  related.  There  are, 
of  course,  differences  in  the  range  of  stimuli  to  which  each  of 
these  forms  of  attitudes  can  be  related,  and  there  are  differences 
in  the  intensity  with  which  these  different  types  of  attitudes  are 
displayed.  There  are  also  differences  due  to  the  conformity  of 
these  attitudes  to  objective  conditions,  as,  for  example,  the  differ- 
ence between  the  attitude  of  a  biologist  to  the  facts  of  biological 
evolution  and  the  attitude  of  an  uneducated  Baptist  to  the  same 
facts.  We  repeat,  however,  that  all  of  these  forms  of  attitudes 
follow  essentially  the  sam^pattern  in  their  development. 

As  commonly  used  the  term  "set"  is  applied  to  a  relatively 
restricted  temporary  attitude,  or  to  momentary  states  of  readiness; 
the  word  "stereotype"  is  applied  to  an  intense  and  rigid  attitude, 
while  the  word  "prejudice"  applies  to  an  attitude  still  more  rigid 
and  intense  and  one  generally  based  on  false  information.  The 
term  "opinion"  is  generally  used  to  describe  an  attitude  that  is  or 
has  been  expressed  and  that  is  based  more  on  objective  conditions 
than  a  "stereotype"  or  "prejudice." 

We  are  mentioning  the  fact  that  these  terms  are  special  cases  of 
attitudes  for  a  methodological  reason.  For  some  investigators  use 
one  particular  term  and  deal  with  the  problems  surrounding  it  as 
if  they  were  separate  problems  unrelated  to  the  characteristics  of 
attitude  or  the  conditions  which  develop  attitudes.  We  strongly 
believe  that  we  shall  gain  much  if  we  unify  the  concepts  which 
can  be  really  unified  and  if  we  do  not  use  different  concepts  for 
the  same  problem.  Following  the  rule  of  scientific  parsimony, 
we  shall  try  to  restrict  ourselves  to  as  few  concepts  as  necessary. 

In  this  chapter  we  have  only  cleared  our  way.  We  have  tried 
to  bring  the  problem  of  attitudes  into  clear  focus.  Because  of  the 


REFERENCES  27 

diverse  and  disconnected  approaches  made  to  the  study  of  attitudes 
by  psychologists  and  sociologists,  some  unification  of  the  problem 
is  called  for.  An  understanding  of  attitudes,  as  well  as  an  under- 
standing of  values,  demands  a  unification  of  the  problem  which 
remains  the  same  problem  whether  handled  by  psychologists, 
sociologists,  or  any  other  social  scientists. 

REFERENCES 

1.  ALLPORT,  G.  W.,  Attitudes,  in  A  Handbook  of  Social  Psychology  (C.  Murchi- 

son,  cd.),  Worcester:  Clark  Univ.  Press,  1935,  ch.  17. 

2.  AMES,  A.,  JR.,  Sensations,  Their  Nature  and  Origin,  Dartmouth  Eye  Institute, 

Hanover,  N.  H.,  October  1945. 

3.  BALDWIN,  J.  M.,  Dictionary  of  Philosophy  and  Psychology,  New  York:  Mac- 

millan,  copyright  1901-05,  3  vols. 

4.  BLUMER,  H.,  An  Appraisal  of  Thomas  and  Znaniecly's  The  Polish  Peasant  in 

Europe  and  America t  New  York:   Soc.  Science  Research  Council,   1939, 
Bull.  44. 

5.  BOGARDUS,  E.  S.,  Fundamentals  of  Social  Psychology, ^New  York:  Century,  1931, 

2ded. 

6.  CANTRIL,  H.,  Attitudes  in  the  making,  Understanding  the  Child,  1934,  4,  13-15. 

7. The  social  psychology  of  everyday  life,  Psychol.  Bull,  1934,  31,  297-330. 

8.  = A  psychological  reason  for  the  lag  of  "nonmaterial"  culture  traits,  Soc. 

Forces,  1935, 13,  376-9. 

9. The  effect  of  modern  technology  and  organization  upon  social  behavior, 

Soc.  Forces,  1937,  15,  493-5. 

10.  : and  D.  KATZ,  Objectivity  in  the  social  sciences,  in  Industrial  Conflict:  A 

Psychological  Interpretation   (G.  [W.  Hartmann  and  T.  Newcomb,  cds.), 
New  York:  Cordon,  1939,  9-18. 

11.  FOLSOM,  J.  K.,  Social  Psychology,  New  York:  Harper,  1931. 

12.  HOROWITZ,  E.  L.,  The  development  of  attitude  toward  the  Negro,  Arch.  Psy- 

chol., 1936,  no.  194. 

13.  HUXLEY,  J.,  Science  and  its  relation  to  social  needs,  in  Scientific  Progress  (Sir 

Halley  Stewart  Lecture,  1935),  London:  Allen  &  Unwin,  1935,  ch.  6. 

14.  JAMES,  W.,  Will  to  Believe  and  Other  Essays  in  Popular  Philosophy,  New  York: 

Longmans,  Green,  1896. 

15.  JUNG,  C.  G.,  Psychological  Types,  New  York:  Harcourt,  Brace,  1924. 

16.  KAY,  L.  W.,  Social  norms  as  determinants  in  the  interpretation  of  personal 

experiences,  /.  Soc.  Psychol.,  1944,  19,  359-67. 

17.  KLINEBERG,  O.,  Social  Psychology,  New  York:  Holt,  1940. 

18.  MURPHY,  G.,  and  L.  B.  MURPHY,  Experimental  Social  Psychology,  New  York: 

Harper,  copyright  1931. 
19. and  T.  M.  NEWCOMB,  Experimental  Social  Psychology,  New  York: 

Harper,  copyright  1937. 
20.  NELSON,  E.,  Attitudes:  I.  Their  nature  and  development,  /.  Gen.  Psychol.,  1939, 

21, 367-99. 


28  THE  PROBLEM  OF  ATTITUDES:  UNIFIED 

21.  ORTH,  J.,  Gefuhl  und  Bewusstseinslage,  'Abhand.  Geb.  Padagog.  Psychol.  u. 

Physiol.,  1903. 

22.  SHELDON,  W.  H.,  The  Varieties  of  Temperament,  New  York:  Harper,  1942. 

23.  SHERIF,  M.,  An  experimental  approach  to  the  study  of  attitudes,  Sociomctry, 

1937,  1,  90-8. 

24.  Some  recent  investigations  on  the  psychology  of  attitudes  (in  Turkish), 

Univ.  Ankara  Publications,  Psychology  Series,  II,  1944. 

25.  SPRANGER,  E.,  Lebensformen,  Halle:  Niemeyer,  1927. 

26.  STRAUSS,  A.,  The  concept  of  attitude  in  social  psychology,  /.  Psychol.,  1945,  19, 

329-39. 

27.  THOMAS,  W.  L,  and  F.  ZNANIECKI,  The  Polish  Peasant  in  Europe  and  America, 

Boston:  Badger,  1918,  vol.  1. 

28.  TITCHENER,  E.  B.,  Systematic  Psychology:  Prolegomena,  New  York:  Macmillan, 

1929. 

29.  WARREN,  H.  C.  (cd.),  Dictionary  of  Psychology,  Boston:  Houghton  Mifflin, 

copyright  1934. 

30.  WATT,  H.  J.,  Experimentelle  Beitrage  zu  einer  Theorie  des  Denkens,  'Arch.  /.  d. 

ges.  Psychol.,  1904,  4,  289-436. 


CHAPTER  3 

SOME  EXPERIMENTAL  FACTS  CONCERNING  THE 
BASIS  OF  ATTITUDES 

Attitudes  are  inferred  from  the  reactions  (verbal  or  nonverbal) 
of  man.  When  an  individual  reacts  repeatedly  in  a  characteristic 
way  (positive  or  negative)  in  relation  to  a  certain  stimulus  object, 
we  infer  that  he  has  an  established  attitude  toward  that  stimulus. 
When  a  group  of  individuals  react  repeatedly  in  a  characteristic 
way  to  a  stimulus  situation,  we  infer  that  the  members  of  the 
group  have  an  established  social  attitude  in  relation  to  it.  This 
characteristic  reaction  of  groups  of  people  is  sometimes  called 
"conforming  behavior."  These  conformities  are  discriminatory 
or  selective,  as  all  attitudes  are.  This  means  that  all  attitudinal 
reactions  are  judgmental  activities.  It  is,  therefore,  not  a  mere 
coincidence  that  social  value  judgments  reveal  themselves  in  the 
psychology  of  the  individual  as  established  attitudes.  Whether 
these  discriminative  activities  revealing  attitudes  are  verbally  ex- 
pressed as  short-cut  judgments  of  opinion  or  value,  as  in  logic,  or 
are  expressed  only  in  behavior  does  not  matter  a  bit  psychologically. 

As  we  said  in  the  previous  chapter,  attitudes  always  imply  a 
subject-object  (stimulus-organism)  relationship.  Attitudes  al- 
ways are  related  to  some  object,  person,  group,  or  standardized 
norm.  This  relationship  is  not  innate,  it  is  formed.  In  order  for 
it  to  be  formed,  the  individual  first  has  to  come  into  contact  with 
the  object  (person,  institution,  norm).  This  is  a  perceptual  situa- 
tion. Therefore,  the  first  stage  in  the  actual  formation  of  an  atti- 
tude is  a  perceptual  stage,  with  the  internal  factors  of  the  organism 
and  external  (objective)  factors  of  the  stimulus  situation  coming 
into  play. 

The  term  "attitude"  (used  in  everyday  language  to  denote  an 
established  state  of  readiness)  does  not  express  any  specific  psy- 
chological mechanism.  It  is  a  composite  term,  especially  useful 

29 


30  EXPERIMENTAL  FACTS  CONCERNING  BASIS  OF  ATTITUDES 

to  denote  in  an  empirical  way  an  important  common  ground  be- 
tween psychologists  and  sociologists.  When  it  is  characterized 
psychologically  and  traced  from  the  point  of  view  of  its  formation, 
it  becomes  evident  that  the  psychology  of  attitudes  is  intimately 
related  to  the  psychology  of  perception  and  judgment.  This  is 
true,  no  matter  what  the  motivational  basis  or  diversity  of  content 
of  an  attitude  may  be.  In  the  present  approach  towards  the  psy- 
chology of  attitudes,  we  shall  put  our  emphasis  on  perceptual  and 
judgmental  processes  and  see  how  far  it  can  carry  us.  Here  we 
do  not  need  to  take  sides  in  favor  of  any  learning  theory.  We 
have  already  pointed  out  that  any  final  adequate  psychology  of 
attitude  will  someday  be  linked  closely  with  the  psychology  of 
learning  or  conditioning,  especially  in  accounting  for  the  more 
or  less  enduring  character  and  for  the  range  of  attitudes. 

General  selectivity  of  perception.  The  objective  world  around 
us — rivers,  hills,  trees,  buildings,  and  the  like — is,  of  course,  not 
affected  by  our  perception  of  it.  These  objects  are  there  objec- 
tively, determined  by  physical  laws,  whether  or  not  we  perceive 
or  experience  them.  On  the  other  hand,  what  we  perceive  of  the 
natural  and  social  world  around  us  is  highly  selective,  determined 
by  biological  and  psychological  laws.  Because  of  this  determinis- 
tic selectivity,  men  perceive  or  experience  different  aspects  of  the 
world  around  them.  For  example,  in  the  Dark  Ages,  because  of 
the  fact  that  men  were  so  confined  to  themselves  owing  to  the 
prevailing  social  and  cultural  system  in  which  they  were  living, 
they  paid  little  attention  to  nature.  [8]  They  needed,  among 
others,  a  Rousseau,  the  rebellious  child  of  the  bourgeois  system 
rising  against  the  decaying  feudal  aristocracy  and  clergy,  to  call 
attention  to  his  fellowmen  that  nature  was  around  them  in  all  its 
glory.  Today,  in  India,  for  example,  many  people  are  so  much 
preoccupied  with  their  mystic  ways,  that  they  simply  are  not  con- 
cerned and,  therefore,  do  not  notice  the  beauties  of  nature.  We 
do  not  have  to  resort  to  any  typologies  to  account  for  these  facts, 
as  Jung  does,  for  example.  [25]  In  a  discussion  of  the  "artist's 
frame  of  reference,"  F.  H.  Allport  [1]  has  nicely  pointed  out  that 
the  meaning,  the  unity,  the  beauty  seen  in  a  picture  are  determined 
by  a  "psychological  frame"  and  that  the  psychology  of  "frame  of 
reference"  "goes  back  to  the  simplest  principles  of  human  percep- 


EXPERIMENTAL  FACTS  CONCERNING  BASIS  OF  ATTITUDES  31 

tion."  [5]  At  the  basis  of  this  selectivity  is  the  bipolar  nature  of 
perception:  the  determination  of  perceptions  by  external  and 
internal  factors  following  certain  laws,  and  the  formation  of 
certain  enduring  states  of  readiness. 

The  selectivity  of  perception  is  a  universal  human  phenomenon 
not  confined  to  any  special  cases.  As  Woodworth  puts  it  in  de- 
veloping what  he  terms  "situation  set/'  "the  individual  is  not  an 
unbiased  registering  instrument."  [55]  Anyone  can  easily  cite 
numerous  cases  from  daily  experience  to  prove  that  what  he  ob- 
serves and  notices  around  him  is  a  selective  matter:  what  he  sees 
in  a  strange  city,  what  he  reads  in  his  newspaper,  and  so  on.  It 
would  be  very  useful  to  make  longitudinal  studies  of  perception 
also  and  find  out  through  a  period  of  time  what  actual  items  a 
person  does  perceive  in  his  surroundings.  Such  longitudinal 
studies  would  certainly  reveal  the  highly  selective  nature  of  our 
daily  perceptions  as  determined  by  external  and  internal  conditions. 

Experiments  in  the  psychological  laboratory  require  subjects, 
through  carefully  formulated  "instructions,"  to  observe  precisely 
certain  aspects  of  the  stimulus  presented  and  usually  made  focal 
for  them  in  a  controlled  way.  Already  some  representative  labora- 
tory studies  furnish  evidence  of  the  general  selectivity  of  percep- 
tion. Here  we  refer  to  the  long  line  of  investigation  started  over 
40  years  ago  by  Kiilpe  when  he  ventured  to  study  the  psychology 
of  the  so<alled  higher  mental  processes  against  the  vigorous  pro- 
tests of  his  former  teacher,  Wundt,  who  argued  for  the  completion 
first  of  the  "mental  chemistry"  of  sensory  processes.  In  Kiilpe's 
laboratory  it  became  increasingly  evident  that  the  "set"  assumed 
by  the  observer  as  a  result  of  "instructions"  was  playing  an  im- 
portant role  in  determining  response  to  the  experimental  situa- 
tion. Kiilpe  himself  specifically  undertook  the  study  of  this 
problem.  [30]  In  his  experiments  he  tachistoscopically  presented 
different  stimuli,  such  as  printed  syllables,  about  which  different  as- 
pects or  "dimensions"  could  be  reported,  for  example,  the  number 
of  letters  involved,  the  locations  of  the  colors,  or  the  total  pattern 
composed  by  them.  Kiilpe  found  that  more  items  were  noted 
and  more  correct  judgments  made  by  the  observer  about  that 
aspect  of  the  stimuli  which  had  been  emphasized  by  the  "set" 
produced  as  a  consequence  of  the  initial  instructions;  subjects 


32  EXPERIMENTAL  FACTS  CONCERNING  BASIS  OF  ATTITUDES 

noticed  more  fully  and  in  more  detail  those  aspects  of  the  stimulus 
field  they  were  set  to  observe.  Two  decades  later  Yokoyama  and 
then  Chapman  verified  Kiilpe's  results.  [6,  9] 

In  the  Kiilpe  line  of  experiments,  the  "set"  is  produced  by  the 
instructions  of  the  experimenter.  But  the  internal  factors  ("set" 
in  this  case)  need  not  always  be  due  to  instructional  set.  In  more 
natural  settings  some  motivational  stress,  some  social  pressure,  or 
some  established  norm  in  the  individual  may  and  does  produce 
the  "set"  or  "attitude"  with  which  the  stimulus  field  is  observed. 
Take,  for  example,  the  case  of  the  hungry  man  looking  for  bread 
or  the  case  of  a  lover  waiting  in  a  crowd  for  his  sweetheart. 
Cantril  has  pointed  out  that 

The  literature  of  experimental  psychology  is  filled  with  research 
which  demonstrates  conclusively  that  a  frame  of  reference  is  an  in- 
evitable accompaniment  of  any  series  of  judgments  a  person  may  be 
asked  to  make;  and  that  when  the  situation  is  unique  and  specific,  as 
it  is  frequently  deliberately  designed  to  be  in  the  laboratory,  the  frame 
of  reference  is  a  function  of  the  relationships  discerned  between  the 
series  of  stimuli.  No  matter  how  hard  the  experimentalist,  especially 
the  psychophysicist,  may  try  to  work  on  the  peripheral  level  of  judg- 
ments and  side-step  central  issues,  he  finds  it  almost  impossible  to 
avoid  in  the  laboratory  the  phenomenon  we  are  describing  that  oc- 
curs so  constantly  in  judgments  of  everyday  life.  [7,  24] 

In  his  impressive  series  of  experiments  carried  out  over  a  period 
of  years  (1913-32),  Bartlett  obtained  a  well-integrated  series  of 
results  on  remembering.  [2]  This  classical  work  breaks  down 
the  artificial  classificatory  boundaries  imposed  between  the  func- 
tions of  perceiving,  remembering,  and  even  imagining.  Bartlett 
unanswerably  demonstrated  that  the  perceiving,  imagining,  and 
remembering  of  even  relatively  simple  objects  are  selective — 
influenced  in  an  important  way  by  such  factors  as  the  interests, 
attitudes,  temperament,  of  the  observer.  He  makes  his  experi- 
ments on  perceiving  the  starting  point  of  experiments  on  remem- 
bering, indicating  a  functional  continuity  between  perception  and 
memory.  In  the  experiments  on  perceiving  [2,  14-33]  he  pre- 
sented to  his  observers  simple  designs  and  patterns,  and  complex 
pictorial  material.  His  instructions  were  neutral;  that  is,  he  did 
not  try  to  produce  a  "set"  or  "attitude"  in  the  observers  in  relation 


EXPERIMENTAL  FACTS  CONCERNING  BASIS  OF  ATTITUDES  33 

to  any  given  aspect  of  the  stimulus  field;  in  fact,  he  was  especially 
on  his  guard  "against  the  use  of  suggestion  other  than  that  con- 
veyed by  the  material  itself."  [17  f.]  His  results  and  conclusions 
are  clear-cut.  In  his  own  words: 

Very  rarely  indeed  did  a  subject  thus  differentiate  clearly  between 
a  sensory  image  set  up  by  the  stimulating  object  and  his  interpretation 
of  the  object  itself.  But,  though  they  did  not  realize  it,  the  observers 
were  throughout  constantly  utilizing  an  imaged  setting,  or  bac\- 
ground,  for  their  perceptual  reactions.  [2,  30,  italics  ours] 

Again, 

...  to  perceive  anything  is  one  of  the  simplest  and  most  immediate, 
as  it  is  one  of  the  most  fundamental,  of  all  human  cognitive  reactions. 
Yet  obviously,  in  a  psychological  sense,  it  is  exceedingly  complex,  and 
this  is  widely  recognized.  Inextricably  mingled  with  it  are  imaging, 
valuing,  and  those  beginnings  of  judging  which  are  involved  in  the 
response  to  plant  order  of  arrangement  and  construction  of  presented 
material.  It  is  directed  by  interest  and  by  feeling,  and  may  be  domi- 
nated by  certain  crucial  features  of  the  objects  and  scenes  dealt  with. 
[2,  31,  italics  ours] 

The  more  unstructured  and  vague  the  stimulus  field  is,  the  more 
important  is  the  role  of  set  and  other  factors  not  inherent  in  the 
•stimulus  itself.  In  one  of  a  series  of  experiments  utilizing  the  auto- 
kinetic  phenomenon,  Sherif  instructed  his  subjects  that  the  light 
(which  was  physically  stationary,  of  course)  would  move  to  the 
right  or  to  the  left  as  the  case  may  be.  [41]  The  subjects  with  the 
"set"  produced  by  the  instructions  gave  results  generally  in  har- 
mony with  the  instructed  direction.  Proshansky  and  Murphy 
[38]  used  the  autokinetic  condition  as  one  of  three  types  of  un- 
structured stimuli  (lines  and  weights  were  also  used)  in  a  test  of 
the  hypothesis  that  "perception  develops  positively  in  the  direction 
of  reward."  [296]  Although  their  "experimental  subjects  showed 
.  .  .  significant  shifts  in  estimates  of  lines  and  weights  in  the 
direction  of  the  percepts  which  had  been  rewarded,"  [305]  this 
conclusion  could  not  be  drawn  from  the  autokinetic  data.  The 
authors  arc  careful  to  point  out,  however,  that  "with  the  auto- 
kinetic effect,  it  was  found  during  the  course  of  the  experiment 
that  a  major  source  of  error  was  operating."  [302]  "Such  large 


34  EXPERIMENTAL  FACTS  CONCERNING  BASIS  OF  ATTITUDES 

and  uncontrolled  autokinetic  effects  appeared  in  the  training 
series  as  to  vitiate  the  post-training  data."  [305]  More  recently 
Haggard  and  Rose,  utilizing  the  autokinetic  technique  with  a 
device  of  reward  or  punishment  instead  of  direct  instruction  of 
movement,  obtained  results  similar  to  Sherif  s.  [18] 

From  the  facts,  of  which  the  foregoing  are  typical  examples,  it 
is  plausible  to  conclude  in  a  general  way  that  the  selectivity  of 
attitudes  is  basically  imbedded  in  the  selectivity  of  perception. 
But  this  gives  us  only  our  general  orientation.  We  must  proceed 
further  and  try  to  single  out  more  precisely  the  processes  involved 
in  the  formation  of  an  attitude. 

A  frame  of  reference  is  involved  in  perceptual  and  judgmental 
activity.  It  is  an  established  fact  in  psychology  that  stimuli  do  not 
have  an  absolute  stimulating  value.  A  stimulus  is  experienced, 
perceived,  judged,  and  reacted  to  in  relation  to  other  stimuli,  pres- 
ent or  past,  to  which  it  is  functionally  related.  In  perception  this 
relative  character  of  a  stimulus  emerges  from  its  relationship  in 
the  organized  whole  [29];  in  judgments,  from  its  relationship  to 
other  stimuli  (present  and  past)  which  are  operative  at  the  mo- 
ment. The  concept  of  "member  character"  expresses  the  relative 
nature  of  the  properties  of  any  stimulus  to  which  the  organism  is 
responding  at  the  moment  and  can  be  conveniently  used  to  denote 
the  relative  character  of  any  stimulating  agent  with  respect  to 
simultaneous  or  preceding  stimuli  with  which  it  is  functionally 
related. 

The  term  "frame  of  reference"  is  simply  used  to  denote  the 
functionally  related  factors  (present  and  past)  which  operate  at 
the  moment  to  determine  the  particular  properties  of  a  psycho- 
logical phenomenon  (such  as  perception,  judgment,  affectivity). 
In  1935  and  1936  Sherif  brought  together  a  good  many  experi- 
mental facts  from  various  major  psychological  phenomena  (in- 
cluding perception,  judgment,  affectivity,  memory,  personality) 
indicating  the  way  in  which  a  "frame  of  reference"  is  involved  in 
each  of  them.  [41,  42]  The  scale  of  magnitudes  against  which 
subsequent  stimuli  of  a  similar  kind  are  judged,  the  organized 
perceptual  whole  which  determines  the  particular  relative  proper- 
ties of  its  parts,  the  established  social  status  in  relation  to  which 
responses  to  other  individuals  and  groups  are  shaped  are  all  specific 


EXPERIMENTAL  FACTS  CONCERNING  BASIS  OF  ATTITUDES  35 

cases  of  frames  of  reference.  Unless  we  have  good  reasons  to  argue 
that  frames  of  reference  involved  in  various  psychological  phe- 
nomena have  nothing  to  do  with  one  another,  but  express  psycho- 
logical tendencies  quite  different  in  nature,  such  a  general  concept 
is  needed  to  denote  this  whole  background  of  factors  which  to- 
gether determine  the  relative  nature  of  response.  Because  frames 
of  reference  are  involved  in  all  major  phenomena,  a  more  precise 
definition  of  frame  of  reference  cannot  be  formulated  until  we 
know  more  about  the  frames  of  reference  in  various  specific  cases. 

Specific  cases  of  frames  of  reference  are  most  extensively  elabo- 
rated in  the  fields  of  perception  and  judgment.  Gestalt  psycholo- 
gists worked  out  in  detail  the  psychology  of  perceptual  frames. 
In  fact,  the  whole  emphasis  of  frames  of  reference  in  psychology 
is  mainly  derived  from  them.  All  our  time  and  space  localiza- 
tions, magnitude  perceptions,  form  perceptions,  perceptions  of 
melody  and  harmony  are  referential  affairs.  When  we  say  "up" 
we  mean  "up"  in  relation  to  something  below,  when  we  say  "far" 
or  "near"  we  say  so  in  relation  to  a  starting  point  we  have  in  mind. 

With  the  shifts  of  the  reference  frames  or  points,  perceptual 
relationships  are  altered.  Thus: 

A  book  is  small  and  a  man  is  large.  But  if  a  house  is  large,  then  a 
man  is  small.  And  if  a  book  is  small  and  a  house  is  large,  then  a  man 
is  of  medium  size.  [36,  43] 

Years  ago  Wertheimer  demonstrated  that  a  line  is  experienced  as 
horizontal  or  vertical  in  reference  to  the  position  of  other  things 
in  the  field  of  stimulation:  if  the  observer's  visual  field  is  slanted 
by  means  of  a  mirror,  a  similarly  slanted  object  tends  to  appear 
vertical,  indicating  that  the  position  of  an  object  is  perceived  in  its 
relation  to  the  whole  organized  field.  [53] 

In  our  opinion,  the  psychology  of  these  perceptual  frames  are 
basically  related  to  the  psychology  of  attitudes.  The  forms,  pro- 
portions, and  magnitudes  of  things,  buildings,  tools,  and  the  like, 
in  the  village,  town,  or  city  we  live  in  become  for  us  in  time 
established  anchorages.  When,  subsequently,  in  some  other  town 
or  country  we  face  different  proportions,  forms,  and  magnitude? 
of  things,  we  perceive  them  against  the  whole  background  of  our 
established  frames  or  anchorages.  Consequently  they  seem  to  us 


36  EXPERIMENTAL  FACTS  CONCERNING  BASIS  OF  ATnTUDES 

to  be  too  large,  too  small,  queer,  or  disproportionate  as  the  case 
may  be. 

Extensive  evidence  of  the  referential  nature  of  judgmental  activi- 
ties has  accumulated  during  the  past  decade.  As  we  shall  see  later, 
the  implications  of  this  important  finding  can  be  carried  on  to 
more  or  less  complicated  social  problems.  Chapman  and  Volk- 
mann,  for  example,  in  a  significant  study  on  "A  Social  Deter- 
minant of  the  Level  of  Aspiration"  start  with  the  general  principle 
that  "the  conditions  which  govern  the  setting  of  a  level  of  aspira- 
tion (Anspruchsniveau),  in  the  sense  of  an  estimate  of  one's  future 
performance  in  a  given  task,  may  be  regarded  as  a  special  case  of 
the  effect  upon  a  judgment  of  the  frame  of  reference  within 
which  it  is  executed."  [10,  225]  They  draw  attention  to  the  "gen- 
eral fact  that  all  judgmental  activities  take  place  within  such 
referential  frameworks."  [225]  Convenient  and  concise  sum- 
maries and  discussions  of  the  studies  on  judgment  are  found  in  the 
reports  of  Long  [31],  Rogers  [39],  and  McGarvey  [34].  If  we 
start  with  Hollingworth's  work  on  judgment,  it  becomes  increas- 
ingly evident  that  judgment  to  a  stimulus  in  a  series  is  based  on 
the  whole  background  of  previous  judgments  belonging  to  the 
series  and  affected  by  them  in  definite  ways.  [21,  22]  For  our 
present  problem,  the  important  fact  is  that  judgments  of  stimuli 
shift  according  to  the  background  furnished  by  a  related  series  of 
stimuli;  the  particular  directions  of  these  shifts  need  not  concern 
our  position  in  this  chapter.  Here  it  is  sufficient  for  us  to  call 
attention  briefly  to  certain  aspects  of  Long's  and  Rogers'  represent- 
ative studies  as  they  bear  on  our  present  problem. 

Long  studied  the  effect  of  preceding  stimuli  upon  the  judgments 
of  succeeding  stimuli,  using  series  of  auditory  intensities.  Among 
other  things  he  found  that 

Under  certain  conditions,  stimuli  oppose  each  other  in  such  a  way 
that  a  weak  stimulus  preceded  by  a  strong  one  is  judged  weaker  than 
it  actually  is,  and  vice  versa.  This  is  referred  to  as  contrast  and  its 
presence  has  been  found  in  experiments  employing  a  variety  of  stimuli: 
namely,  tones  and  weights  in  the  usual  psychophysical  experiments; 
and  colors,  tones,  and  odors  in  experiments  on  hedonic  tones.  Thus 
the  reality  of  the  phenomenon  cannot  be  doubted,  but  an  explanation 
of  why  contrast  operates  or  what  processes  (either  psychological  or 


EXPERIMENTAL  FACTS  CONCERNING  BASIS  OF  ATTITUDES  37 

physiological)  underlie  it,  must  be  postponed  until  the  conditions 
under  which  it  occurs,  or  does  not  occur,  are  better  known.  [31,  55] 

The  studies  on  judgment  with  the  method  of  single  stimuli  that 
have  accumulated  during  the  past  15  years  furnish  unequivocal 
evidence  in  support  of  the  relativity  of  judgment.  In  an  increasing 
number  of  studies  in  different  sense  modalities,  it  has  been  shown 
that  the  use  of  a  standard  stimulus  is  not  necessary  for  the  observer 
to  give  a  judgment  about  any  stimulus  in  the  series.  After  a  few 
rounds  of  presentation  of  the  series,  observers  spontaneously  estab- 
lish a  scale  without  being  instructed  to  do  so.  Any  stimulus  in 
the  series  is  judged  or  placed  in  its  relative  position  in  the  scale. 
These  experiments  in  Which  no  standard  stimulus  is  used  yield  a 
distribution  of  frequencies  of  judgment  very  similar  to  that  ob- 
tained by  orthodox  psychophysical  methods  using  a  formal  stand- 
ard. [37] 

Here  again  we  see  the  same  referential  nature  of  response 
observed  in  cases  of  perception.  With  the  shifts  of  the  reference 
frame  or  scale  to  which  any  stimulus  is  related,  a  corresponding 
shift  in  judging  that  stimulus  results.  Wever  and  Zener  [54] 
gave  an  observer  a  "light"  series  of  weights,  and  after  this  series 
had  become  an  "established"  scale  for  the  observer  they  suddenly 
introduced  a  "heavy"  series. 

The  effect  of  the  first  series  on  the  judgments  of  the  second  was 
quite  evident  for  20  or  25  presentations,  i.e.  for  four  or  five  rounds 
judgments  of  "heavy"  predominated  for  all  the  stimuli;  from  this 
point  on,  however,  the  judgments  showed  a  redistribution  conforming 
to  the  second  stimulus-series.  [54,  476,  italics  ours] 

On  the  basis  of  these  facts,  we  may  say  that  the  members  of  a 
series  of  stimuli  manifest  "member-character"  just  as  parts  of  an 
organized  perceptual  field  manifest  "member-character,"  revealing 
"supra-local"  qualities  from  the  functional  relationship  in  which 
they  are  found.  The  member-character  of  the  stimuli  in  a  series 
is  clearly  shown  in  a  keen  observation  of  Wedell  in  his  study  of 
pitch  discriminations.  . 

It  has  been  said  that  no  judgment  is  "absolute"  unless  a  long  time, 
say  12  hours,  has  intervened  since  the  last  hearing  of  a  tone.  Follow- 
ing this  line  of  reasoning  in  the  present  experiment,  the  only  absolute 


38  EXPERIMENTAL  FACTS  CONCERNING  BASIS  OF  ATTITUDES 

judgment  would  be  the  first  one  each  day.  It  must  be  admitted  that 
there  is  some  basis  for  this  assertion,  because  some  of  the  subjects 
seemed  to  compare  the  notes  with  one  another  deliberately.  In  addi- 
tion to  this,  a  subject  would  sometimes  correct  a  previous  judgment 
by  saying,  "Oh,  that  other  one  must  have  been  [so  and  so]."  [51,  497, 
italics  ours] 

Psychophysical  studies  dispensing  with  the  presentation  of  a 
formal  standard  stimulus  with  every  variable  stimulus  in  the  series 
were  first  started  by  Wever  and  Zener.  [54]  Because  no  standard 
was  used,  and  each  stimulus  was  presented  singly,  the  method  was 
designated  as  the  method  of  "single"  or  "absolute  stimuli/'  and  the 
series  of  stimulus  values  as  "absolute  series."  Now  it  is  often 
referred  to  as  "absolute  scale"  and  the  judgments  obtained  as 
"absolute"  judgments.  It  is  clear  that  the  term  "absolute"  refers 
to  the  method  alone.  "Absolute"  does  not  refer  to  judgments  or 
their  distribution.  The  real  psychological  fact  shown  in  all  these 
experiments  is  that  judgments  to  subsequent  stimuli  are  relative 
to  preceding  stimuli  of  a  similar  nature.  Referring  to  the  general- 
ity of  this  psychological  tendency,  McGarvey  states  in  her  survey 
of  literature  in  1943  that 

The  relativity  of  judgments  of  lifted  weights,  tonal  pitches,  visual 
inclinations,  etc.,  is  paralleled  by  the  relativitv  of  affective  judgments. 
[34, 14] 

It  is  an  unfortunate  accident  to  have  the  term  "absolute" 
connected  with  a  whole  array  of  facts  which  definitely  reveal  the 
relativity  of  judgment,  perception,  and,  in  a  word,  experience  in 
general.  As  the  term  "absolute"  is  used  in  a  contradictory  way  to 
designate  the  relative  nature  of  a  general  psychological  tendency, 
there  is  bound  to  appear  a  contradiction  of  terms.  Thus  Cohen, 
in  a  study  following  Beebe-Center's  research  showing  the  relativity 
of  hedonic  judgments,  uses  the  contradictory  title  "The  Relativity 
of  Absolute  Judgments."  [12]  The  same  contradiction  of  terms 
is  found  in  a  more  recent  study  by  Postman  and  Miller  showing 
the  relative  nature  of  temporal  judgments.  The  authors  state: 

An  "absolute"  judgment  of  magnitude  is,  of  course,  not  strictly 
absolute,  but  is  formed  in  relation  to  other  magnitudes  that  lie  within 
the  immediate  universe  of  attention.  [36,  43] 


EXPERIMENTAL  FACTS  CONCERNING  BASIS  OF  ATTITUDES  39 

Again, 

Such  subjective  scales  have  been  called  absolute  scales  and  perhaps 
the  term  is  no  more  contradictory  than  the  term  absolute  judgment. 
Absolute,  in  these  contexts,  means  relative  to  the  comparable  magni- 
tudes that  form  the  immediate  context  of  the  judgment.  [36,  43,  the 
last  emphasis  is  ours] 

To  designate  a  tendency  by  a  contradictory  term  does  not  help  to 
clarify  but  only  to  confuse  the  issues  in  question.  It  may  astound 
people  to  see  that  psychologists  designate  the  same  thing  as  both 
"absolute"  and  "relative." 

It  seems  to  us  that  nothing  is  gained  by  stretching  the  term 
"absolute  judgment"  and  "absolute  scale"  (already  used  in  a  con- 
tradictory way  by  the  mere  accident  of  naming  a  method)  to  study 
the  relative  nature  of  response  in  perceptual  and  social  fields.  The 
relative  nature  of  response  in  sensory  judgments  is  only  a  specific 
case  of  a  more  general  tendency.  For  example,  contrast  and  as- 
similation effects  which  necessarily  appear  in  judgments,  are  not 
effects  peculiar  to  judgments  alone.  Assimilation  and  contrast 
effects  result  from  simultaneous  stimulation  (perception)  as  well 
as  from  successive  stimulation,  as  can  be  easily  found  by  con- 
sulting any  psychology  textbook.  Not  only  are  contrast  and 
assimilation  effects  simultaneous  as  well  as  successive — but  also 
they  are  different  according  to  the  place  they  occupy  in  the  figure 
or  ground  of  a  perceptual  relationship.  [4]  In  view  of  these  facts, 
appropriate  specific  terms  should  be  used  to  designate  specific  cases 
of  reference  frames  appearing  in  other  major  fields  of  psychology. 
For  example,  "aspiration  level"  and  "ego  level"  may  be  used  as 
such  referential  terms  in  cases  dealing  with  the  experiences  of 
success,  failure,  and  status,  as  Hoppe  [23],  Frank  [16],  Gould 
[17],  and  others  have  done. 

In  this  connection,  Rogers'  significant  study  dealing  with  the 
anchoring  effect  of  a  preceding  stimulus  of  constant  value  on  a 
scale  of  stimuli  is  pertinent.  His  formulation  is  aptly  offered  "as 
an  experimental  contribution  to  the  understanding  of  the  frame 
of  reference."  [39]  His  study  is  based  on  a  previous  investigation 
reported  by  Volkmann  in  1936.  [48]  Volkmann  obtained  judg- 
ments on  a  series  of  visual  inclinations,  using  the  method  of 


40  EXPERIMENTAL  FACTS  CONCERNING  BASIS  OF  ATTITUDES 

single  stimuli.  In  the  first  experiment  an  "instructed"  position 
(horizontal)  was  used  as  a  reference  point.  As  a  consequence,  the 
scale  shifted  and  extended  considerably  in  the  direction  of  the 
horizontal.  The  second  experiment  demonstrated  that  in  a  simi- 
lar manner  any  value  which  the  observer  selected  and  held  in 
mind  could  exert  an  appreciable  shift  in  the  scale. 

In  the  first  part  of  his  study  Rogers  discusses  frame  of  reference 
and  "absolute  scale."  He  almost  characterizes  the  "mental  forma- 
tion," labeled  "absolute  scale"  in  an  inconsistent  way,  as  a  special 
case  of  frame  of  reference.  ("Mental  formation"  was  originally 
used  by  Wever  and  Zener  to  designate  the  psychological  scale 
[frame]  formed  after  some  rounds  of  presentation  of  the  stimulus 
series  in  these  psychophysical  experiments.)  Rogers  states: 

While  it  might  be  argued  that  the  absolute  scale,  in  this  event,  con- 
stitutes a  frame  of  reference  accessible  to  experimental  manipulation 
and  investigation,  a  slightly  different  view  is  to  be  taken  here.  The 
frame  of  reference,  it  would  seem,  may  most  reasonably  be  seen  as  the 
product  of  a  number  of  influences,  notably  the  influences  of  stimuli 
which  cannot  all  be  present  at  any  one  time.  [39,  6] 

This  is  true  in  a  general  way,  but  not  quite,  because  the  influence 
of  past  stimuli,  not  present  at  the  time,  may  be  operative  on  an 
"absolute"  scale  as  well  as  on  any  other  specific  case  of  a  reference 
frame.  For  example,  in  building  up  a  mental  scale  ("absolute 
scale")  using  the  method  of  single  stimuli,  a  previously  established 
scale  or  anchorage  even  from  daily  life  may  come  in  to  modify 
present  judgments.  The  operation  of  a  frame  of  reference  or  a 
mental  scale  as  a  specific  case  of  a  frame  of  reference  is  detected 
from  the  observable  determinations  or  modifications  of  reported 
judgments  and  perceptions.  This  fact  is  clearly  noted  by  Rogers: 

The  frame  of  reference,  then,  is  inaccessible  except  in  terms  of  overt 
responses  which  it  governs.  [39,  6] 

In  the  experiments  which  Rogers  conducted,  he  used  the  method 
of  single  stimuli.  His  stimulus  series  consisted  of  a  scale  of  visual 
inclinations  (in  the  first  experiment)  and  a  scale  of  weights  (in 
the  second  experiment).  In  Rogers'  first  sessions  the  usual  dis- 
tributions of  judgments  were  obtained,  and  the  usual  scales  (or 
frames)  were  established  psychologically.  In  subsequent  sessions, 


EXPERIMENTAL  FACTS  CONCERNING  BASIS  OF  ATTITUDES  41 

"an  anchoring  stimulus"  was  presented  just  prior  to  each  presen- 
tation of  a  stimulus  to  be  judged  and  was  designated  as  the  top 
category  of  the  series.  The  anchoring  stimulus,  at  first  the  same 
as  the  highest  stimulus  of  the  range,  "was  moved  progressively 
further  above  the  stimulus  range,  remaining  always  the  same 
throughout  any  single  session."  This  means  that  the  most  fre- 
quently presented  stimulus  was  the  anchoring  stimulus:  to  be  pre- 
cise, the  anchoring  stimulus  was  presented  as  many  times  as  the 
total  number  of  presentations  of  all  the  stimuli  of  the  series.  In  a 
few  cases,  the  values  of  the  anchoring  stimuli  used  were  within  the 
upper  part  of  the  stimulus  range.  Rogers'  results  obtained  from 
both  kinds  of  stimuli  (visual  inclinations  and  weights)  are  similar. 
The  anchoring  or  reference  point,  experimentally  introduced, 
produces  changes  in  the  scale  and  in  the  category  thresholds  within 
the  scale.  As  the  anchoring  point  moves  further  from  the  range, 
it  expands  the  scale  to  a  certain  point,  and  as  it  is  carried  down 
into  the  range  it  causes  the  scale  to  shrink.  Briefly  stated,  the  scale 
(frame)  expands,  to  a  certain  point,  or  shrinks,  but  in  both  cases 
it  is  assimilated  to  the  shifts  of  the  anchoring  stimulus. 

Rogers'  investigations  constitute  a  significant  contribution  to  the 
systematic  experimental  study  of  frames  and  points  of  reference 
and  to  the  nature  of  their  mutual  interdependence.  Our  daily 
perceptions,  experiences,  relationships  with  other  individuals  are 
structured  or  altered  to  an  important  degree  by  the  conscious  or 
unconscious  use  of  intruded  anchorages  of  a  social  or  nonsocial 
nature.  Thus  when  we  say  "early,"  "late,"  we  say  them  in  relation 
to  certain  reference  points,  for  example,  the  time  of  the  departure 
of  a  train,  an  appointment,  lunch  time.  In  social  life,  we  shift  our 
judgments,  decisions,  and  human  relationships  by  self-imposed  or 
socially  given  intrusions  of  value  judgments  which  serve  us  as 
anchorages.  In  his  work  on  remembering,  Bartlett  gives  vivid 
illustrations  of  how  the  names  of  things  used  at  the  moment  serve 
as  anchorages  in  relation  to  which  perceptions  and  memories  are 
structured  or  altered. 

A  recent  study  by  Tresselt  [45]  is  of  special  importance  for  us 
in  this  connection,  because  it  provides  a  strict  laboratory  demon- 
stration of  the  fact  that  the  better  an  anchorage  is  learned  (even 
to  the  extent  of  being  overlearned),  the  more  pronounced  is  the 


42  EXPERIMENTAL  FACTS  CONCERNING  BASIS  OF  ATTITUDES 

tendency  for  that  anchorage  to  effect  subsequent  judgments. 
Tresselt  set  out  "to  determine  the  effect  of  experimentally  pro- 
duced variations  in  the  amount  of  past  experience  (with  stimulus- 
objects  similar  to  the  objects  to  be  used  as  the  new  task  of  judg- 
ment) upon  the  speed  of  agreement  with  the  center  of  the 
stimulus-scale."  She  used  a  series  of  12  weights,  ranging  from  11 
to  560  grams.  Six  groups  of  subjects  participated  in  the  experi- 
ment. Group  I  was  given  one  practice  trial  on  each  of  the  four 
heaviest  weights;  group  II  was  given  4  trials  on  each  of  the  heavi- 
est weights;  group  III  was  given  8  trials;  group  IV  12;  group  V 
28;  and  group  VI  12  practice  trials  on  the  four  lightest  weights. 
As  usual  in  such  experiments,  Tresselt  found  that 

By  the  time  the  subjects  have  lifted  approximately  six  weights  re- 
gardless of  the  amount  of  previous  experience  or  kind  of  previous 
experience,  the  judgments  are  less  widely  distributed  and  it  might  be 
said  that  the  subjects  have  come  to  show  a  greater  degree  of  conformity 
about  what  shall  be  called  "medium."  [45] 

But  she  continues: 

The  larger  the  period  of  practice,  the  more  slowly  does  the  scale  of 
judgment  shift  to  its  new  position.  [45,  italics  ours] 

The  more  well-established  an  anchorage  is,  the  better  it  is  learned, 
then  the  more  lasting  is  its  influence  likely  to  be. 

The  shifts  and  other  effects  brought  about  by  the  introduction 
of  anchorages  into  a  situation  (anchorages  lying  within  or  without 
the  structure)  are  demonstrated  by  facts  accumulating  almost  since 
the  beginnings  of  experimental  psychology.  Thus,  Henri,  work- 
ing on  skin  localizations  in  1892-97,  found  that  shifts  of  localiza- 
tions always  "are  committed  in  the  direction  of  the  points  of 
reference  (points  de  reptre)"  and  corresponding  shifts  took  place 
with  shifts  of  reference  points.  [20]  What  Henri  called  reference 
points  (points  de  reptrc)  in  1895  were  designated  "anchoring 
points"  by  Koffka  in  1922  [27],  "anchoring  agents"  by  Volkmann 
in  1936  [48],  and  "anchoring  points"  by  Rogers  in  1941  [39]  in 
studying  the  same  effects. 

Before  proceeding  to  our  next  step — the  experimental  formation 
of  an  attitude— we  shall  bring  together  the  main  points  reached  so 


STRUCTURED  AND  UNSTRUCTURED  STIMULUS  SITUATIONS  43 

far.  The  first  stage  in  the  formation  of  an  attitude  is  a  perceptual 
stage.  Because  of  this  and  because  of  the  discriminative  nature  of 
attitudes  they  ate  closely  linked  to  the  psychology  of  perception 
and  judgment.  The  laboratory  studies  on  one  hand  and  historical 
and  empirical  facts  of  everyday  life  on  the  other  reveal  that  per- 
ceptions are  selective.  Perceptual  and  judgmental  activities  take 
place  in  referential  frameworks.  As  a  consequence  of  facing  re- 
peatedly the  proportions,  forms,  or  perceptual  objects,  scales  of 
magnitudes  (both  in  a  physical  and  a  social  sense),  these  scales 
and  magnitudes  form  frames  of  reference  in  the  individual  which 
serve  as  bases  by  which  subsequent  situations  are  perceived  and 
judged.  They  need  not  be  consciously  formed,  deliberately  in- 
structed, or  imposed  by  others.  Once  formed  they  act  as  anchor- 
ages to  determine  or  alter  an  individual's  reactions  to  subsequent 
situations.  In  this  fact  is  imbedded  the  basic  psychology  of  an 
attitude. 

And  lest  we  be  accused  of  too  much  subjectivity,  it  should  be 
mentioned  in  passing  that  experiments  concerning  the  referential 
nature  of  judgments  involving  manual  tasks  are  entirely  feasible. 
For  example,  Tresselt  reports  that  in  her  experiment  with  Volk- 
mann  involving  lifted  weights  they  gathered  data  which  "show 
that  one  subject  who  worked  in  a  steel  plant  held  the  weights  to 
be  light  or  medium  for  the  first  eight  judgments  while  a  college 
professor  held  all  the  weights  to  be  heavy  or  medium,  except  one, 
the  very  lightest."  [45]  Proshansky  and  Murphy  have  pointed 
out  that  "the  unity  of  the  organism  would  suggest  .  .  .  that  what- 
ever principles  are  of  value  in  the  understanding  of  the  laws  of 
motor  response  are  of  some  pertinence  in  understanding  the  laws 
of  perception."  [38,  295] 

FRAMES  OF  REFERENCE  IN  RELATION  TO  STRUCTURED  AND 
UNSTRUCTURED  STIMULUS  SITUATIONS 

We  shall  first  consider  frames  of  reference  in  relation  to  struc- 
tured stimulus  situations,  beginning  with  the  most  clear-cut  cases 
of  perception.  When  the  stimulus  field  is  well-structured,  the 
grouping  or  organization  that  follows  gives  rise  to  perceptions  of 
forms,  magnitudes,  melodies,  rhythms,  proportions,  relationships, 


44  EXPERIMENTAL  FACTS  CONCERNING  BASIS  OF  ATTITUDES 

localizations,  and  so  on,  that  correspond,  in  general,  to  the  proper- 
ties of  the  objective  situations:  the  perception  of  a  circle,  a  square, 
an  appropriately  grouped  succession  of  tones  typifj?  these  situations. 
In  such  cases  the  structure  of  the  psychological  frame  will  cor- 
respond closely  to  the  structure  of  the  external  field  of  stimulation, 
the  figure-ground  relationship  being  determined  by  the  compel- 
ling features  and  salient  reference  points  of  the  objective  situation. 
The  properties  of  the  different  parts  are  determined  by  their  func- 
tional relationship  in  the  structure.  This  is  expressed  as  the  mem- 
ber-character of  the  parts.  Factors  in  the  objective  field  of  stimula- 
tion (such  as  proximity  and  similarity)  with  such  compelling 
features  have  been  studied  extensively  since  the  outstanding  work 
of  Wertheimer.  [52]  This  member-character  relationship  holds 
true  with  successive  stimulation  as  is  the  case  in  the  perception  of 
melody  as  well  as  with  simultaneous  stimulation.  As  we  have 
seen,  this  seems  to  be  true  in  the  case  of  judgments  as  well  as  of 
perceptions. 

Well-structured  objects  or  magnitudes  have  similar  effects  in 
our  daily  life  reactions,  as  we  have  already  noted.  An  individual, 
wherever  he  may  be,  is  surrounded  by  buildings,  tools,  furniture, 
magnitudes,  timetables,  schedules,  or  innumerable  other  types  of 
well-structured  stimulus  situations  of  one  kind  or  another.  He  is 
stimulated  by  them  repeatedly.  As  a  consequence,  the  particular 
structures,  magnitudes,  and  relationships  become  the  established 
scales  or  frames  in  him.  This  objective  determination,  by  existing 
magnitudes,  scales,  relationships,  of  lasting  scales  or  frames  in 
individuals  implies  the  establishment  of  lasting  norms.  These 
norms,  verbalized  or  nonverbalized  in  explicit  judgments,  are  fun- 
damental in  shaping  the  mentalities  of  individuals  living  in  any 
social  system.  They  are,  in  fact,  at  least  as  important  in  shaping 
the  mentality  of  members  of  a  society  as  value  judgments,  beliefs, 
or  the  whole  superstructure  of  culture.  Spiritually  inclined  culture 
apologists  have  tended  to  ignore  or  minimize  the  effects  of  this 
basic  field  of  determination. 

Now  we  shall  consider  frames  of  reference  in  relation  to  un- 
structured stimulus  situations.  We  have  ample  evidence  that  in 
cases  where  the  stimulus  situation  is  not  well-structured  the  result- 
ing psychological  experience  is  by  no  means  always  chaos  or  an 


STRUCTURED  AND  UNSTRUCTURED  STIMULUS  SITUATIONS  45 

inconsistent  hodgepodge  of  reactions.  It  seems  that  a  tendency  to 
organize  and  group  stimuli  is  a  primary  psychological  fact  based, 
of  course,  on  underlying  properties  of  the  nervous  system  which 
competent  physiologists  will  no  doubt  someday  explain.  Even  in 
cases  where  the  stimulus  field  is  not  well-structured  and  does  not 
have  the  properties  necessary  to  impose  objectively  clear-cut  non- 
reversible  figure-ground  relationships,  there  is  usually  some  sort  of 
organization.  For  example,  campers  in  a  forest  on  a  dark  night 
are  apt  to  see  or  hear  different  things  around  them  as  determined 
by  their  individual  attitudes  or  preoccupations.  But  in  such  cases, 
internal  factors  are  important  in  determining  the  properties  of  the 
resulting  organizations. 

The  margin  of  possibilities  for  the  contribution  of  internal  fac- 
tors allowed  by  unstructured  situations  is  at  the  basis  of  many 
studies  dealing  with  the  problem  of  individual  peculiarities,  per- 
sonality differences,  abnormal  tendencies,  and  the  like.  We  find 
here  the  basis  of  the  projective  devices  currently  flourishing.  The 
use  of  unstructured  ink  blots  for  detecting  characteristics  of  the 
individual  was  suggested  and  used  by  G.  V.  Dearborn  back  in  the 
end  of  the  19th  century.  [13,  14]  Others  soon  followed  him. 
Bartlett  used  a  series  of  ink  blots  in  his  studies  on  remembering. 
The  recent  extensive  systematic  uses  of  projective  methods  based 
on  the  Rorschach  ink  blots  or  Murray's  thematic  apperception  test 
are  well  known.  [26,  35]  Voth  recently  studied  personality  differ- 
ences "as  expressed  through  various  forms  and  amount  of  auto- 
kinetic  perception"  using  autokinesis  as  an  unstructured  unstable 
stimulus.  [49]  In  more  recent  studies,  Voth  has  used  the  auto- 
kinetic  phenomenon  as  an  index  to  pathological  tendencies  and 
has  found  fairly  high  correlations  between  indices  thus  obtained 
from  his  patients  and  medical  diagnosis.  [50] 

Gradations  of  structure  in  the  stimulus  field.  We  have  con- 
sidered cases  of  structured  and  unstructured  stimulus  situations: 
in  the  former  cases  the  resulting  psychological  outcome  is  com- 
pellingly  determined  by  the  objective  situation,  in  the  latter  cases 
a  variety  of  internal  or  subjective  factors  come  into  play  to  shape 
response.  Actually,  of  course,  there  are  all  kinds  of  gradations 
between  these  two  extremes  of  structuration. 

Recently  Luchins  used  gradations  of  structure  or  ambiguity  in 


46  EXPERIMENTAL  FACTS  CONCERNING  BASIS  OF  ATTITUDES 

a  series  of  studies  on  the  social  influences  involved  in  the  percep- 
tion of  complex  drawings.  [33]  The  conclusion  that  can  be  drawn 
from  these  studies  is  that  the  effects  of  social  influence  (the  various 
devices  of  suggestion  used  in  the  experiment)  vary  with  the  de- 
gree of  ambiguity  of  stimuli  presented.  The  greater  the  ambiguity 
of  the  stimulus,  the  greater  is  the  effect  of  attempted  social  influ- 
ence. In  a  previous  study  in  which  the  stimulus  gradations  were 
too  few  and  too  abrupt  to  allow  the  possibility  of  graded  compari- 
sons, Luchins  seems  to  reach  a  similar  conclusion: 

Whether  or  not  subjects  were  influenced  by  A's  judgment  ["A" 
being  the  influencing  subject  in  the  experiment]  seemed  to  depend  on 
the  obviousness  of  the  correct  answer,  i.e.,  the  clarity  of  the  judgment- 
situation,  on  the  truth  or  falsity  of  A's  judgment,  and  also  on  the 
subjects'  attitudes  to  and  interpretations  of  their  task  and  the  experi- 
mental situation.  [32,  110,  italics  ours] 

Coffin  designed  an  experiment  to  study  the  relationship  of 
suggestibility  to  the  ambiguity  of  a  stimulus  situation.  The  tonal 
attributes  of  pitch,  volume,  and  a  fictitious  attribute  created  for 
the  experiment  and  labeled  "orthosonority"  were  used  as  the  three 
tonal  attributes  varying  in  ambiguity.  Subjects  were  given  a  tonal 
stimulus  and  then,  after  each  tonal  dimension  had  been  defined, 
were  told  to  equate  the  succeeding  tone  heard  through  their  head- 
phones with  the  original  stimulus  by  turning  the  appropriate  dial 
which  was  ostentatiously  labeled.  Subjects  were  divided  into  ex- 
perimental and  controlled  groups.  Results  showed  that  the  least 
ambiguous  tonal  attribute,  pitch,  was  in  most  cases  not  subject  to 
change  by  suggestion.  Volume,  on  the  other  hand,  could  be 
reversed  by  suggestion  with  most  observers,  while  judgments  of 
"orthosonority"  invariably  followed  the  experimenter's  suggestion. 
In  other  words  suggestibility  to  these  attributes  increases  with  their 
ambiguity.  [11] 

In  a  study  dealing  with  social  determinants  of  the  level  of 
aspiration  Chapman  and  Volkmann  produced  changes  in  the 
standards  of  their  subjects  by  introducing  into  the  situation  a 
hierarchy  of  standards  the  truth  or  falsity  of  which  could  not  be 
objectively  tested  by  the  subjects.  But  in  a  subsequent  experiment, 
after  the  subjects  had  considerable  experience  with  the  task  at 


STRUCTURED  AND  UNSTRUCTURED  STIMULUS  SITUATIONS  47 

hand,  the  introduction  of  new  standards  did  not  shift  the  standards 
of  the  subjects.  With  such  results  in  mind,  Chapman  and  Volk- 
mann  state  that  "the  lability  of  the  judgment,  for  example,  varies 
inversely  with  the  determinateness  of  the  frame  of  reference." 
[10,  225]  As  we  shall  see  later,  Asch  and  his  associates  got  similar 
results.  CantriFs  studies  of  a  panic  situation  and  various  social 
movements  substantiate  the  implications  of  these  results  on  a 
highly  complicated  level,  [p.  81  /.]  Although  research  is  still  scanty 
which  deals  directly  with  the  comparative  effects  on  perception 
and  judgment  of  gradations  of  structuration  in  the  stimulus  field, 
from  the  meager  results  obtained  thus  far  we  may  formulate  as  a 
working  hypothesis  that,  all  other  things  being  equal,  the  role 
played  by  internal  and  social  factors  decreases  with  the  stability, 
clarity,  or  structuredness  of  the  stimulus  situation  and  with  the 
strength  of  frames  or  points  of  reference  already  established. 

As  we  have  remarked  in  the  foregoing,  material  objects,  techno- 
logical and  other  products  of  human  labor,  have  a  compelling 
effect  in  producing  corresponding  frames  in  the  psychology  of 
men.  If  the  actual  truth  of  all  man's  individual  and  social  relation- 
ships were  compellingly  imposed  on  him  in  daily  life,  then  he 
would  not  have  any  lasting  and  organized  false  attitudes.  But  he 
actually  does  have  false  attitudes  about  things,  especially  those  he 
has  never  seen  for  himself  or  closely  scrutinized.  He  has  definite 
attitudes  about  gods,  about  life  hereafter,  and  imposed  attitudes 
concerning  peoples  about  whom  he  really  knows  nothing. 

As  formulations  and  standards  have  been  achieved  in  science 
through  the  laborious  and  not  infrequently  persecuted  labors  of 
scientific  workers  such  as  Galileo,  science  has  changed  and  cor- 
rected many  obsolete  "survival"  social  norms  and  corresponding 
attitudes.  False  norms  and  practices  connected  with  natural  events 
have  been  corrected  and  have  yielded  their  place  to  scientific  for- 
mulations in  the  modern  world,  although  there  are  exceptions 
such  as  the  norms  of  Christian  Science.  However,  in  social  and 
economic  relationships  and  in  the  whole  superstructure  of  norms 
determined  by  them,  many  survivals  remain.  Until  social  systems 
base  their  premises  and  practices  on  strictly  scientific  grounds, 
"survival"  norms  and  attitudes  filtering  down  from  different  his- 
torical periods  and  outmoded  systems  will  continue  to  function. 


48  EXPERIMENTAL  FACTS  CONCERNING  BASIS  OF  ATTITUDES 

The  social  system  that  erects  itself  on  a  scientific  basis  is  still  the 
exception  rather  than  the  rule.  In  view  of  this  fact,  the  psycholo- 
gist renders  a  real  service  when  he  takes  a  realistic  stand  and 
studies  human  nature  and  social  systems  as  they  really  are,  rather 
than  basing  his  arguments  on  the  inherent  goodness  or  evil  of 
man  or  the  will  to  truth  as  Hobbes  and  Rousseau  did  in  their 
times  and  as  the  advocates  of  an  unchanging  "human  nature"  are 
doing  today.  The  more  various  interest  groups  strive  to  perpetuate 
obsolete  superstructures  of  norms,  the  more  important  it  is  to  study 
social  organization  objectively.  Many  a  historian  and  social  sci- 
entist has  observed  that  unstable,  ambiguous,  and  critical  situations 
are  those  which  provide  especially  fertile  soil  for  the  formation  and 
inculcation  of  social  norms.1 

In  the  following  paragraphs  we  shall  try  to  typify  experiment-, 
ally  the  basic  psychological  processes  involved  in  the  formation  of 
frames  in  unstructured  situations.  Admittedly,  the  studies  are 
carried  on  in  artificial  and  miniature  laboratory  situations.  In 
presenting  them  we  claim  only  that  they  are  a  starting  point  for 
conceptualization,  the  validity  of  which  will  be  tested  out  with 
more  concrete  social  material  in  actual  situations. 

Formation  of  a  frame  in  an  unstructured  situation?  In  line 
with  the  previous  discussion,  the  psychologist  must  find  an  un- 
stable unstructured  stimulus  situation,  present  it  repeatedly  to  the 
individual  to  see  if  he  will  structure  it  somehow  to  a  framework 
of  response — proceeding  with  the  hypothesis  that  psychological 
organization  or  grouping  is  a  primary  fact  in  unstructured  as  well 
as  in  structured  situations.  And  if  stimuli  are  organized  by  the 
individual,  then  the  further  question  is,  will  they  be  organized  by 
the  members  of  a  group  collectively,  and  will  members  of  a  group 

1  It  might  be  pointed  out  here  that  no  realistic  purpose  is  served  by  conducting 
experiments  of  the  type  in  which  the  attempt  is  made,  by  introducing  various 
social  influences,  to  have  a  subject  perceive  a  perfect  square  as  a  circle,  or  a  line 
two  inches  long  as  longer  than  a  line  four  inches  long.    In  such  obvious  cases,  no 
problem  is  involved,  and  nothing  is  gained  by  declaring  as  a  conclusion  in  the 
face  of  such  a  compelling  stimulus  determination,  "Ah  ha,  people  cannot  be 
influenced  to  perceive  a  square  as  a  circle;  thus  they  reveal  their  inherent  will  to 
truth." 

2  When  not  otherwise  specified,  the  results  to  be  summarized  here  refer  to 
Sherif.  [41,  42]    These  references  may  be  consulted  for  the  detailed  description 
of  the  experimental  situation,  procedure,  subjects,  instructions,  and  so  on. 


STRUCTURED  AND  UNSTRUCTURED  STIMULUS  SITUATIONS  49 

eventually  form  a  frame  peculiar  to  the  group?  This  query  lies 
at  the  basis  of  the  psychology  of  what  sociologists  such  as  Durk- 
heim,  drawing  a  sharp  dichotomy  between  individual  psychology 
and  social  psychology,  argue  so  strongly  concerning  the  supralocal, 
suis  generis  character  of  collective  values  or  "representations" 
which  emerge  only  as  a  consequence  of  collective  behavior.  [15] 

The  autokinetic  phenomenon  is  one  of  various  experimental 
possibilities  that  conveniently  lends  itself  to  a  test  of  frame  forma- 
tion when  the  stimulus  is  unstructured.  In  a  pitch-dark  space  a 
single  point  of  light  seems  to  move  and  to  move  in  different 
directions.  It  seems  to  move  because  there  is  no  frame  of  reference 
to  give  it  a  stable  localization.  With  the  introduction  of  other 
visible  points  or  objects  the  point  gains  stability  relative  to  these 
points,  since  all  psychological  localizations  are  relative  affairs. 
Even  the  introduction  of  sounds  in  the  vicinity  of  the  point  seems 
to  affect  its  stability. 

The  first  experiment  in  the  series  studied  the  formation  of  a 
frame  (or  scale)  in  the  individual  alone,  thus  starting  with  the 
general  psychology  of  frame  formation.  The  stimulus  light  was 
presented  briefly  and  successively  one  hundred  times  in  each  ex- 
perimental session.  The  time  of  exposure  after  the  perceived  move- 
ment started  was  the  same  in  all  presentations.  This  was  true  for 
all- sessions,  individual  and  group.  The  observer  was  asked  to 
report  the  extent  of  the  perceived  movement.  The  results  un- 
equivocally indicate  that,  even  though  a  scale  and  reference  points 
are  lacking  in  the  objective  situation,  in  the  course  of  the  experi- 
mental session  individuals  spontaneously  form  a  frame  and  a 
central  tendency  (standard)  which  may  differ  from  individual  to 
individual  in  the  absence  of  a  compelling  objective  range  of 
stimuli.  In  other  words,  in  the  absence  of  an  objective  scale 
(frame)  and  objective  standard  (reference  point)  each  individual 
builds  up  a  scale  of  his  own  and  a  standard  within  that  scale. 
The  range  and  reference  point  established  by  each  individual  is 
peculiar  to  himself  when  he  is  facing  the  situation  alone. 

In  the  second  part  of  this  first  experiment  it  was  found  that  once 
a  scale  is  established  there  is  a  tendency  for  the  individual  to  pre- 
serve this  scale  in  subsequent  sessions  (within  a  week  in  these 
experiments).  The  introspective  data  obtained  furnish  further 


50  EXPERIMENTAL  FACTS  CONCERNING  BASIS  OF  ATTITUDES 

evidence  of  the  formation  of  a  frame.  Most  typical  examples  of 
such  introspection  are:  "Compared  with  previous  distance";  "Judg- 
ments are  all  relative";  "Compared  successive  judgments";  "First 
estimate  as  standard."  Although  the  subjects  do  form  frames  of 
reference  of  their  own  spontaneously  without  being  instructed  to 
do  so,  the  lack  of  an  objective  frame  of  reference  is  experienced. 
The  following  introspections  are  typical:  "Darkness  left  no  guide 
for  distance";  "Lack  of  visible  neighboring  objects";  "No  fixed 
point  from  which  to  judge  distance." 

SOCIAL  FACTORS  IN  LABORATORY  SITUATIONS 

We  can  now  proceed  to  the  consideration  of  attitudes  in  social 
situations.  These  situations  range  from  laboratory  experiments 
involving  social  factors  (which  we  will  consider  here)  to  compli- 
cated social  conditions  of  everyday  life  (to  be  taken  up  in  the  next 
chapter).  In  doing  so,  we  should  stress  again  our  conviction  that, 
in  spite  of  the  enormous  variation  in  the  content  of  attitudes, 
psychological  principles  of  attitude  formation  are  essentially  the 
same,  irrespective  of  what  the  attitude  is  concerned  with. 

The  first  stage  of  attitude  formation — in  the  most  complicated 
social  situation  as  well  as  in  a  restricted  laboratory  experiment — is 
a  perceptual  stage.  The  individual  must  somehow  come  into  con- 
tact with  the  stimulus  situation  before  any  attitude  is  established. 
In  strict  laboratory  situations  this  stimulus  situation  is  generally 
neutral,  and  the  frame  of  reference  established  lacks  the  vital 
affective  and  motivational  properties  so  characteristic  of  social 
attitudes:  in  the  laboratory  experiment  the  stimuli  may  be  a  series 
of  magnitudes,  such  as  lines  or  weights  or  perceptual  structures; 
in  social  life  the  stimuli  may  be  a  person,  group,  an  expressed  atti- 
tude or  prejudice,  a  threat,  or  a  value  judgment  (norm)  of  any 
kind.  Some  of  these  stimuli  in  social  life  may  be  relatively  neutral, 
may  have  only  a  mild  affective  property,  for  example,  the  color  of 
a  dress,  the  shape  of  a  house,  the  characterization  of  a  city,  while 
some  may  involve  intense,  sometimes  even  traumatic,  experiences, 
for  example,  a  girl  whom  one  loves  at  first  sight,  a  scolding 
received  by  a  child  for  cheating,  an  announcement  over  the  radio 
that  the  Japanese  have  bombed  Pearl  Harbor. 


SOCIAL  FACTORS  IN  LABORATORY  SITUATIONS  51 

In  dealing  with  attitudes  in  social  situations,  then,  we  are  deal- 
ing with  stimulus  situations  rife  with  affective  and  motivational 
properties,  with  situations  that  frequently  involve  problems  of  the 
individual's  status.  It  might  be  mentioned  in  passing  that  attitudes 
to  situations  involving  affective  properties  seem  to  be  more  readily 
established  (learned),  just  as  Pavlov,  Tolman,  and  others  showed 
that  conditioning  was  more  effective  under  motivational  stress. 
Whether  the  perceptual  experience  involves  direct  contact  with  an 
object,  person,  or  group,  or  whether  the  perceptual  experience 
involves  the  verbal  transmission  of  a  social  norm  by  some  short- 
cut value  judgment  makes  no  difference  whatever  as  far  as  the 
basic  psychological  characteristics  of  attitude  formation  are  con- 
cerned. Both  the  soldier  who  has  had  direct  experience  with  the 
enemy  and  his  sister  back  home  who  has  been  subjected  to  value 
judgments  concerning  the  enemy  can  and  often  do  develop  vari- 
ous negative  attitudes.  It  is  therefore  systematically  useless  to  try 
to  categorize  attitudes  according  to  their  source  or  origin.  This 
is,  of  course,  not  a  denial  of  the  specific  properties  of  particular 
attitudes  formed  in  specific  situations. 

Experimental  formation  of  a  frame  in  group  situations.  This 
series  of  experiments  was  undertaken  with  the  methodological 
conviction  that  the  principles  formulated  and  verified  in  general 
psychology  are  valid  principles  that  operate  when  individuals  par- 
ticipate in  group  action  or  in  collective  behavior.  Results  pro- 
duced by  group  interaction  may  be,  and  in  fact  are,  different  from 
those  obtained  when  a  person  is  alone.  They  may  have  all  the 
earmarks  of  emergent  products.  Nevertheless,  the  psychological 
principles  involved  in  the  group  situation  are  the  same  in  opera- 
tion as  those  when  an  individual  is  not  a  member  of  a  group. 
The  emergence  of  new  qualities  is  not  a  unique  property  of  group 
action  alone— perceptions  of  forms,  relationships,  and  melodies 
emerge  as  unique  qualities  not  found  in  individual  parts.  This 
emergence  of  new  qualities  on  different  levels — individual  as  well 
as  collective  levels— has  to  be  stressed  constantly  because  of  the 
sharp  dichotomies  drawn  by  certain  schools  of  thought.  For  ex- 
ample, the  dichotomy  of  psychology  into  experimental  versus 
cultural  made  by  the  romantic  German  school  represented  by 
Spranger  [44]  portrays  experimental  (individual)  psychology  as 


52  EXPERIMENTAL  FACTS  CONCERNING  BASIS  OF  ATTITUDES 

atomistic  and  static,  and  cultural  psychology  as  dynamic  and 
meaningful  with  total  qualities.  Writers  of  the  Durkheim  school 
of  spiritualistic  sociology,  for  example,  Blondel  [5]  and  Halb- 
wachs  [19],  and  Durkheim  [15]  himself,  argue  that  new  quali- 
ties emerge  only  in  collective  situations,  whereas  findings  in  psy- 
chology substantiate  the  view  of  dialectical  materialism  that 
emergence  takes  place  in  all  levels — physical,  biological,  psycho- 
logical and  socioeconomic.  In  a  recent  study  full  of  implications 
for  further  research  in  social  psychology,  Tresselt  and  Volkmann 
experimentally  demonstrated  the  "production  of  uniform  opinion 
by  non-social  stimulation."  [47]  These  investigators  express  a 
general  psychological  fact  when  they  state  that  "the  mechanism  for 
judging  social  stimuli  is  the  same  as  the  mechanism  for  judging 
non-social  stimuli."  [242] 

With  this  methodological  concern  in  mind,  we  can  turn  to  the 
results  of  Sherif's  group  experiments  on  autokinesis.  In  the  group 
situations  each  subject  reported  his  judgments  aloud  and  naturally 
was  heard  by  the  other  subjects  in  the  group.  He  in  turn  heard 
their  expressed  judgments.  No  primacy  effect  was  introduced 
into  the  situation.  A  special  point  was  made  in  the  wording  of 
the  instructions  to  the  effect  that  subjects  could  report  their  judg- 
ments in  any  order  in  any  presentation.  A  subject  who  gave  his 
judgment  first  in  a  particular  presentation,  might  be  the  last  to 
report  in  the  following  presentation.  The  building  up  of  a  frame 
in  group  situations  is  a  temporal  affair  established  in  the  course 
of  the  experimental  period— the  influence  of  expressed  judgments 
is  not  restricted  to  judgments  given  at  particular  presentations. 

Some  groups  faced  the  autokinetic  situation  without  any  pre- 
vious individual  experience.  An  equal  number  of  groups  faced  it 
after  the  members  had  established  their  individual  frames  alone  in 
individual  experiments.  The  members  of  the  groups  who  first 
took  part  directly  in  the  group  situation  were  therefore  naive  and 
neutral  in  relation  to  this  unstructured  ambiguous  situation. 

The  results  are  clear.  When  individuals  as  members  of  a  group 
face  the  same  unstable  unstructured  situation  without  any  pre- 
viously established  personal  relationship  among  them,  a  scale 
(frame)  and  a  standard  (reference  point)  within  that  frame  are 


SOCIAL  FACTORS  IN  LABORATORY  SITUATIONS  53 

established  in  the  course  of  the  experimental  period.8  When  a 
member  of  the  group  faces  the  same  situation  subsequently  done, 
after  the  frame  is  established  in  the  group  situation,  he  experi- 
ences the  situation  in  terms  of  the  frame  established  in  the  group 
situation  as  a  consequence  of  group  interactions. 

This  result  was  verified  by  Asch  and  Wright  in  experiments 
performed  in  1937.  We  give  these  results  here  in  Asch's  own  pre- 
ferred terminology.4 

The  outstanding  feature  of  the  investigation  of  Sherif  is  the  tend- 
ency of  the  subjects  in  the  given  experimental  situation  to  reach 
mutual  agreement.  S.  E.  Asch  and  B.  Wright  undertook  in  a  series  of 
experiments  to  examine  more  closely  the  specific  reasons  for  this 
tendency. 

Before  proceeding  with  their  experimental  variations,  they  first 
repeated  the  main  experiment  of  Sherif  with  four  pairs  of  subjects. 
The  arrangement  of  the  experiment  was  as  far  as  possible  identical 
with  that  of  Sherif.  The  main  findings  of  Sherif  were  confirmed: 
a  new  norm  (level  of  response)  developed  under  the  given  conditions. 
However,  the  convergence  proceeded  along  different  paths  in  the 
different  pairs  of  subjects.  For  example,  some  members  exercised 
more  influence  on  the  direction  than  others.  This  finding  is  entirely 
consistent  with  the  results  of  Sherif.  [Variations  of  these  experiments 
by  Asch  and  Wright  will  be  reported  in  their  appropriate  context 
later.] 8 

In  Sherif 's  experiments  with  autokinesis,  when  individuals  first 
establish  their  frames  and  standards  in  individual  sessions  and  are 

8  Here  we  shall  not  digress  to  indicate  some  interesting  results  which  revealed 
the  differential,  relative  contributions  of  individual  members  and  which  are  im- 
portant for  the  psychology  of  personality. 

4  This  is  a  summary  statement  of  investigations,  the  full  account  of  which  will 
be  reported  in  a  forthcoming  publication  by  Dr.  S.  Asch.    The  results  reported 
here  were  personally  communicated. 

5  More  recently,  in  a  series  of  studies  dealing  with  "some  social  factors  in  judg- 
ment," Schonbar  investigated  modifications  of  judgments  in  "situations  of  medium 
and  high  structure,"  respectively.    [40]     From  her  results  she  concludes  "our 
findings  confirm  and  extend  the  conclusions  arising  from  Sherif's  work  on  the 
autokinetic  phenomenon."  [729]    That  the  convergence  she  found  in  cases  of  high 
structure  is  almost  identical  to  the  convergence  found  in  cases  of  medium  structure 
is  puzzling,  in  view  of  the  differentiated  results  obtained  with  varied  degrees  of 
structure,  and  other  factors  by  Asch,  Lewis  and  Hertzman,  Coffin,  Chapman  and 
Volkmann,  Luchins,  and  Asch  and  Wright 


54  EXPERIMENTAL  FACTS  CONCERNING  BASIS  OF  ATTITUDES 

then  brought  into  group  situations,  their  judgments  tend  to  con- 
verge. But  the  convergence  is  not  so  close  as  when  they  first  work 
in  the  group  situation.  When  an  individual  comes  into  a  group 
situation,  with  his  own  established  frames  and  standards,  there  is 
a  tendency  to  stick  to  them  to  some  extent:  individuals  bring  into 
the  situation  their  own  established  frames  and  are  no  longer  naive 
in  relation  to  the  situation.  [41,  31,  41;  42, 104] 

In  one  variation  of  these  experiments,  Asch  and  Wright  gave 
contradictory  instructions  in  the  individual  sessions  by  prescribing 
varying  ranges  of  movement. 

Under  these  conditions  of  contradictory  individual  norms,  no 
convergence  developed  in  any  of  the  pairs.  Subsequent  questioning 
revealed  that  fully  one-half  of  the  subjects  did  not  realize  that  they 
were  in  contradiction  with  their  partners.  They  interpreted  the  addi- 
tion of  the  integer  3  (or  the  dropping  of  the  integer  3)  as  introduced 
for  the  sake  of  identifying  each  of  the  subjects  in  the  dark  room.  The 
remaining  subjects  interpreted  the  existing  difference  in  terms  of  such 
factors  as  differences  of  position  with  regard  to  the  light,  differences 
in  eyesight,  etc. 

This  means  that  even  following  individual  sessions,  with  pre- 
scribed contradictory  instructions  and  contradictory  standards  and 
norms,  half  of  the  subjects  converged.  The  addition  or  dropping 
of  the  integer  "3"  (which  the  subjects  thought  was  introduced  as 
an  experimental  device)  accounts  for  statistical  but  not  for  psy- 
chological nonconvergence  as  explained  by  the  subjects  who  "did 
not  realize  that  they  were  in  contradiction  with  their  partners." 
In  a  more  decisive  variation  aimed  at  the  complete  destruction 
of  convergence,  Asch  and  Wright  performed  the  following  experi- 
ment. In  Asch's  words: 

The  aim  of  the  following  experiment  was  to  alter  in  a  decisive  way 
the  cognitive  character  of  the  situation.  The  subjects  were  informed 
in  the  individual  session  concerning  the  subjective  nature  of  the  auto- 
kinetic  effect  (each  subject  was  paired  in  the  present  variation  with  a 
"planted"  subject  whose  estimates  differed  considerably  and  consist- 
ently from  the  experimental  subject's).  There  were  ten  subjects  in 
this  experiment.  Under  these  conditions,  five  subjects  showed  no 
convergence  effect  whatsoever;  the  other  five  did  depart  significantly 
from  their  previous  judgments  and  in  the  direction  of  the  "planted" 


SOCIAL  FACTORS  IN  LABORATORY  SITUATIONS  55 

subject.  To  explain  the  two  forms  of  reaction,  the  subjects  were 
questioned  at  the  conclusion  of  the  investigation.  According  to  the 
present  results,  it  seems  that  the  subjects  who  showed  the  convergence 
effect  continued  to  think  of  the  autokinetic  phenomenon  as  objective 
and  forgot  the  preceding  instructions.  Nevertheless,  the  change  found 
in  the  first  half  of  the  group  conclusively  demonstrates  how  effective 
the  alteration  of  the  cognitive  character  of  the  situation  can  be.  [italics 
ours] 

In  spite  of  the  fact  that  the  subjects  were  told  beforehand  that  the 
light  was  not  moving  at  all  and  in  spite  of  their  individually  es- 
tablished norms  before  coming  into  the  group  situation,  it  is  sur- 
prising that  there  was  any  degree  of  convergence  at  all.  We  do 
not  know  if  the  50  per  cent  convergence  attributed  by  Asch  to 
forgetting  is  in  line  with  current  forgetting  curves.  The  lapse  of 
time  between  the  individual  and  group  sessions  cannot  be  longer 
than  a  few  days. 

Nevertheless  the  afore-mentioned  variations  which  introduced 
into  the  situation  various  degrees  of  familiarity  or  objective  knowl- 
edge of  the  situation  account  for  the  variations  in  the  results 
obtained.  They  are  in  harmony  with  Sherifs  results  which 
showed  that,  once  individual  frames  are  established,  convergence 
or  agreement  in  subsequent  group  situations  is  decidedly  affected 
by  them.  Likewise,  the  findings  in  these  variations  are  in  har- 
mony with  the  conclusions  of  Chapman  and  Volkmann  that  pre- 
vious familiarity  with  a  situation  prevents  shifts  in  the  direction 
of  experimentally  introduced  standards  or  norms.  Likewise,  they 
are  in  harmony  with  the  finding  of  Luchins  that  social  influence 
varies,  among  other  factors,  with  variations  in  structural  clarity. 
And  all  are  in  harmony  with  the  formulation  reached  in  the 
previous  section  that  the  influence  of  internal  and  social  factors 
increases  directly  with  the  increase  of  the  unstructuredness  or  am- 
biguity of  the  situation,  and  decreases  with  the  degree  of  the 
structuredness  or  unambiguity  of  frames  and  standards  already 
established  by  an  individual. 

Experimental  inculcation  of  a  frame.  As  we  stressed  previously, 
frames  are  formed  in  the  individual  in  daily  life  corresponding  to 
objects  and  norms  around  him  which  are  compelling  in  their  ob- 
jective structure.  Quite  frequently  attitudes  are  formed  as  a  direct 


56  EXPERIMENTAL  FACTS  CONCERNING  BASIS  OF  ATTITUDES 

consequence  of  short-cut  verdicts  or  value  judgments  of  grown-ups, 
teachers,  and  others  around  us.  These  value  judgments  may  or 
may  not  be  imbedded  in  the  truth  of  actual  relationships.  We 
are  concerned  here  with  the  psychological  process  involved  in  their 
acquisition.  The  formation  of  such  a  frame  in  a  social  situation 
has  been  experimentally  demonstrated.  The  experiment  was  car- 
ried out  by  Sherif  in  the  spring  of  1936  at  Columbia  University 
again  utilizing  the  autokinetic  technique,  [43] 

The  specific  problem  in  the  experiment  was  to  see  whether 
"naive"  subjects  could  be  experimentally  inculcated  with  varying 
ranges  of  prescribed  frames  and  standards.  Preliminary  experi- 
mentation had  shown  that  a  partner  in  this  situation  (in  this  case 
a  partner  with  considerable  prestige)  could  influence  the  judg- 
ments of  the  other  subject — raise  and  lower  them  in  his  direction. 
When  the  "naive"  subject  was  told  exactly  what  had  happened 
she  became  very  disturbed,  an  indication  that  an  individual  can 
get  emotionally  upset  by  being  so  fooled.  [92] 

In  the  main  experiment  there  were  seven  groups  of  two  mem- 
bers each.  In  each  group,  one  member  co-operated  with  the  ex- 
perimenter by  deliberately  distributing  his  judgments  around  the 
range  (scale)  and  a  standard  point  within  that  range  prescribed 
beforehand  by  the  experimenter.  The  second  member  (desig- 
nated as  the  "naive"  subject)  was  totally  unfamiliar  with  the  situa- 
tion. In  each  case  the  naive  subject  was  not  acquainted  either 
with  the  experimenter  or  the  co-operating  subject.  To  be  sure 
that  the  conformity  was  to  the  prescribed  range  and  standard,  a 
different  range  and  a  different  standard  were  prescribed  for  each 
group.  In  order  to  avoid  the  factor  of  primacy,  the  co-operating 
subject  was  instructed  to  let  the  "naive"  subject  express  his  judg- 
ment first  at  least  half  the  time.  The  first  session  was  the  group 
session  with  both  the  co-operating  and  "naive"  subjects  participat- 
ing. In  the  second  session  the  "naive"  subject  was  alone. 

The  results  indicate  considerable  convergence  to  the  prescribed 
range  (scale)  and  standard  in  the  judgments  of  the  "naive"  sub- 
jects. The  varying  convergence  in  different  subjects  clearly  indi- 
cates personality  differences.  The  convergence  of  the  judgments 
in  the  "alone"  session,  which  took  place  the  day  following  the 
group  session,  was,  in  general,  even  greater  than  that  obtained  in 


REFERENCES  57 

the  group  sessions.  Introspections  reveal  that  the  subjects  became 
conscious  of  the  scale  and  standard  formed  during  the  course  of 
the  experiments:  Five  subjects  out  of  seven  reported  in  their  writ- 
ten introspections  at  the  end  of  the  experiments  that  they  were 
not  influenced  by  the  judgments  of  their  partner.  This  clearly 
indicates  that  a  social  influence  which  continues  even  when  the 
individual  is  alone  need  not  be  consciously  experienced.  Schonbar 
has  recently  obtained  results  which  lead  to  the  same  conclu- 
sion. [40] 

It  is  a  significant  fact  to  find  convergence  greater,  in  the  majority 
of  cases,  when  the  individual  is  acting  alone.  It  is  not  a  rare  oc- 
currence in  everyday  life  to  react  negatively  or  hesitatingly  on 
some  topic  raised  by  some  person  while  in  his  presence,  but  to 
respond  positively  when  he  is  no  longer  in  the  situation.  To  yield 
easily  is  not  a  pleasant  "ego"  experience. 

This  experiment  embodies  in  itself  the  rudiments  of  the  psy- 
chology of  attitude  formation:  a  scale  (or  frame)  is  formed  under 
the  influence  of  the  verdicts  of  another  individual  in  relation  to 
a  definite  experimentally  controlled  stimulus  situation.  The  scale 
(frame)  is  carried  from  one  day  to  another,  has  to  some  extent  an 
enduring  quality  which  provides  an  individual  with  a  state  of 
readiness  by  means  of  which  he  reacts  in  a  characteristic  way  to 
the  same  stimulus  situation.  Here  we  have  the  main  earmarks 
of  an  attitude.  We  must  admit  at  once  that  this  formation  lacks 
the  affective  quality  of  a  real  attitude.  But  we  do  not  believe  this 
at  all  invalidates  our  conceptualization.  The  investigations  of 
Beebe-Center  [3]  and  associates,  Cohen  [12],  and  Hunt  and  Volk- 
mann  [24]  all  indicate  that  the  mechanisms  of  affective  judg- 
ments are  not  essentially  different  from  those  of  other  judgments. 
Besides,  as  we  shall  see,  there  is  further  evidence  to  support  the 
general  scheme  so  far  presented. 

REFERENCES 

1.  ALLPORT,  F.  H.,  The  psychology  of  the  artist's  frame  of  reference,  unpublished 

talk  to  Associated  Artists,  Syracuse,  N.  Y.,  April  17,  1945.    (Outline  of  talk 
personally  communicated  to  the  authors.) 

2.  BARTLETT,  F.  C.,  Remembering:  A  Study  in  Experimental  and  Social  Psychol- 

ogy, Cambridge  (England) :  Univ.  Press,  copyright  1932. 


58  EXPERIMENTAL  FACTS  CONCERNING  BASIS  OF  ATTITUDES 

3.  BEEBE-CENTER,   J.    G.,   Pleasantness  and   Unpleasantness,   New   York:    Van 

Nostrand,  1932, 

4.  DENARY,  W.,  The  influence  of  form  on  brightness  contrast,  in  A  Source  Eoo\  of 

Gestalt  Psychology  (W.  D.  Ellis,  ed.),  New  York:  Harcourt,  Brace,  1938, 
104-08. 

5.  BLONDEL,  C.,  Introduction  a  la  psychologic  collective,  Paris:  Colin,  1928. 

6.  BORING,  E.  G.,  Attribute  and  sensation,  Am.  ].  PsychoL,  1924,  35,  301-04. 

7.  CANTRIL,  H.,  The  Psychology  of  Social  Movements,  New  York:  Wiley,  copy- 

right 1941. 

8.  CHANDLER,  A.  R.,  Beauty  and  Human  Nature,  New  York:  Appleton-Century, 

1934. 

9.  CHAPMAN,  D.  W.,  Relative  effects  of  determinate  and  indeterminate  Aujgaben, 

Am.  /.  PsychoL,  1932,  44,  163-74. 

10. and  J.  VOLKMANN,  A  social  determinant  of  the  level  of  aspiration,  /.  Ab- 

norm.  &  Soc.  PsychoL,  1939,  34,  225-38. 

11.  COFFIN,  T.  E.,  Some  conditions  of  suggestion  and  suggestibility:  a  study  of 

certain  attitudinal  and  situational  factors  influencing  the  process  of  sugges- 
tion, PsychoL  Monogr.,  1941,  no.  241. 

12.  COHEN,  N.  E.,  The  relativity  of  absolute  judgments,  Am.  J.  PsychoL,  1937,  49, 

93-100. 

13.  DEARBORN,  G.  V.,  Blots  of  ink  in  experimental  psychology,  PsychoL  Rev.,  1897, 

4,  390-1. 
14. A  study  of  imaginations,  Am.  J.  PsychoL,  1898,  9,  183-90. 

15.  DURKHEIM,  E.,  Les  Formes  elementaires  de  la  vie  religieuse,  Paris,  1912. 

16.  FRANK,  J.  D.,  Some  psychological  determinants  of  the  level  of  aspiration,  Am. 

L  PsychoL,  1935,  47,  285-93. 

17.  GOULD,  R.,  An  experimental  analysis  of  "level  of  aspiration,"  Genet.  PsychoL 

Monogr.,  1939,21,  1-116. 

18.  HAGGARD,  E.,  and  G.  J.  ROSE,  Some  effects  of  mental  set  and  active  participa- 

tion in  the  conditioning  of  the  autokinetic  phenomenon,  /.  Exp.  PsychoL, 
1944,  34,  45-59. 

19.  HALBWACHS,  M.,  Les  Cadres  sociaux  de  la  memoire,  Paris,  1925. 

20.  HENRI,  V.,  Recherches  sur  la  localisation  des  sensations  tactiles,  Annee  PsychoL, 

1895,  2,  168-92. 

21.  HOLLINGWORTH,  H.  L.,  The  inaccuracy  of  movement,  Arch.  PsychoL,  1909, 

no.  13. 
22. The  central  tendency  of  judgment,  /.  Phil.,  1910,  7,  461-9. 

23.  HOPPE,  F.,  Erfolg  und  Misserfolg,  PsychoL  Forsch.,  1930,  14,  1-62. 

24.  HUNT,  W.  A.,  and  J.  VOLKMANN,  The  anchoring  of  an  affective  scale,  Am.  ]. 

PsychoL,  1937,  49,  88-92. 

25.  JUNG,  C.  G.,  Psychological  types,  New  York:  Harcourt,  Brace,  1924. 

26.  KLOPFER,  B.,  and  D.  M.  KELLY,  The  Rorschach  Technique,  Yonkers:  World 

Book,  1942. 

27.  KOFFKA,  K.,  Perception:  an  introduction  to  the  Gestalt-Theorie,  PsychoL  Bull., 

1922,  19,  531-85. 
28. Principles  of  Gestalt  Psychology,  New  York:  Harcourt,  Brace,  1935. 

29.  KOHLER,  W.,  Gestalt  Psychology,  New  York:  Liveright,  1929. 

30.  KU'LPE,  O.,  Versuche  iiber  Abstraktion,  Ber.  I.  Kongr.  Exp.  PsychoL,  1904, 

58-68. 


REFERENCES  59 

31.  LONG,  L.,  A  study  of  the  effect  of  preceding  stimuli  upon  the  judgment  of 

auditory  intensities,  Arch.  PsychoL,  1937,  no.  209. 

32.  LUCHINS,  A.  S.,  On  agreement  with  another's  judgments,  /.  Abnorm.  &  Soc. 

PjycAo/.,  1944,39,97-111. 

33. Social  influences  on  perception  of  complex  drawings,  /.  Soc.  PsychoL, 

1945,  21,  257-73. 

34.  McGARVEY,  H.  R.,  Anchoring  effects  in  the  absolute  judgment  of  verbal  ma- 

terials, Arch.  PsychoL,  1943,  no.  281. 

35.  MURRAY,  H.  A.,  et  al.t  Explorations  in  Personality,  New  York:  Oxford,  1938. 

36.  POSTMAN,  L.,  and  G.  A.  MILLER,  Anchoring  of  temporal  judgments,  Am.  /. 

PsychoL,  1945,  58,  43-53. 

37.  PRATT,  C.  C.,  Time-errors  in  the  method  of  single  stimuli,  /.  Exp.  PsychoL, 

1933,  16,  798-814. 

38.  PROSHANSKY,  H.,  and  G.  MURPHY,  The  effects  of  reward  and  punishment  on 

perception,  /.  PsychoL,  1942,  13,  295-305. 

39.  ROGERS,  S.,  The  anchoring  of  absolute  judgments,  Arch.  PsychoL,  1941,  no.  261. 

40.  SCHONBAR,  R.  A.,  Some  social  factors  in  judgment:  the  formation  of  social 

norms  in  the  judgment  of  structured  stimuli  and  esthetic  preference,  Ph.D. 
thesis,  1945,  Columbia  Univ.  Library. 

41.  SHERIF,  M.,  A  study  of  some  social  factors  in  perception,  Arch.  Psychol.t  1935, 

no.  187. 
42. The  Psychology  of  Social  Norms,  New  York:  Harper,  copyright  1936. 

43.  An  experimental  approach  to  the  study  of  attitudes,  Sociometry,  1937,  1, 

90-8. 

44.  SPRANGER,  E.,  Lebensformen,  Halle:  Niemeyer,  1927. 

45.  TRESSELT,  M.  E.,  The  influence  of  amount  of  practice  upon  the  formation  of 

a  scale  of  judgment,  to  be  published  in  /.  Exp.  PsychoL 
46. The  effect  of  the  experiences  of  contrasted  groups  upon  the  formation 

of  a  new  scale  of  judgment,  to  be  published. 
47. and  J.  VOLKMANN,  The  production  of  uniform  opinion  by  nonsocial 

stimulation,  /.  Abnorm.  &  Soc.  PsychoL,  1942,  37,  234-43. 

48.  VOLKMANN,  J.,  The  anchoring  of  absolute  scales,  PsychoL  Bull.,  1936,  33,  742-3. 

49.  VOTH,  A.,  Individual   differences  in  the  autokinetic  phenomenon,  /.   Exp. 

PsychoL,  1941,  29,  306-22. 

50. Psychologist's  report,  in  33rd  biennial  report  (Topeka  State  Hospital), 

1942,  16-20. 

51.  WEDELL,  C.  H.,  The  nature  of  the  absolute  judgment  of  pitch,  /.  Exp.  PsychoL, 

1934,  17,  485-503. 

52.  WERTHEIMER,  M.,  Untersuchungen  zur  Lehre  von  der  Gestalt,  PsychoL  Forsch., 

1922, 1,  47-58. 
53. DM  Abhandlungen  zur  Gestalttheorie,  Erlangen:  Phil.  Akad.,  1925. 

54.  WEVER,  E.  G.,  and  K.  E.  ZENER,  The  method  of  absolute  judgment  in  psycho- 

physics,  PsychoL  Rev.,  1928,  35,  466-93. 

55.  WOODWORTH,  R.  S.,  Situation-and-goal  set,  in  Am.  ].  PsychoL,  Golden  Jubilee 

vol.  (K.  M.  Dallenbach,  ed.),  1937, 50, 130-40. 


CHAPTER  4 

THE  ATTITUDE  FORMULATION  EXTENDED 
TO  CONCRETE  SITUATIONS 

The  conceptual  scheme  offered  here  towards  the  psychology  of 
attitudes  may  appear  to  be  an  "artifact''  based  only  on  the  results 
of  laboratory  experiments  which  have  no  counterparts  in  real  life 
situations.  So  we  turn  now  to  some  examples  of  more  everyday 
life  situations  which  seem  to  us  to  confirm  the  reality  of  our 
formulations.  We  cite  here  only  a  few  of  the  many  studies,  rang- 
ing from  relatively  simple  demonstrations  to  complex  reactions 
of  daily  life,  that  confirm  the  conclusions  reached  in  the  more 
strictly  laboratory  settings.1 

Irrespective  of  the  particular  way  in  which  an  individual  ac- 
quires an  attitude  in  social  life,  the  literature  of  social  psychology 
is  rife  with  data  which  support  the  formulations  reached  from 
our  survey  of  general  psychology:  that  perception  and  judgment 
are  selective  and  occur  within  a  referential  framework,  that  frames 
or  points  of  reference  are  inevitably  established  if  an  individual 
repeatedly  faces  the  same  stimulus  situation,  that  these  frames  and 
points  of  reference  are  by  no  means  always  confined  to  consciously 
accepted  instructions  or  imposed  norms  but  can  become  estab- 
lished without  the  individual's  realization  of  it,  and  that  once 
established  these  frames  and  points  of  reference  serve  as  anchor- 
ages for  perception  and  judgment. 


might  incidentally  call  attention  here  to  the  fact  that  most  experiments  on 
fcumans  in  the  psychological  laboratory  are  social  situations  at  least  to  the  extent 
that  they  involve  verbal  material  in  the  instructions  given  to  subjects.  The  variety 
of  possible  meanings  of  stimulus  situations  persistently  gives  trouble  since  meanings 
so  readily  intrude  themselves.  [38,  76,  94]  It  is  well  known  that  a  subject's  "set" 
or  "attitude"  can  profoundly  affect  his  reactions  in  psychological  experiments. 
It  may  also  be  remembered  that  Titchener,  in  trying  to  create  the  "proper"  set, 
advocated  the  use  of  "trained  observers."  From  our  present  vantage  point  these 
results  obtained  only  from  observers  trained  to  rule  out  objective  references  seem 
highly  artificial 


ATTITUDE  FORMULATION  EXTENDED  TO  CONCRETE  SITUATIONS      61 

It  should  be  pointed  out  that,  whereas  scales  and  frames  are 
experimentally  constructed  in  the  laboratory,  when  we  are  deal- 
ing with  concrete  social  material  it  is  generally  necessary  to  select 
some  range  of  stimuli  which  is  meaningful  in  the  particular  situa- 
tion we  are  concerned  with.  If  some  limits  were  not  generally 
established  by  the  social  organization,  it  would  be  much  more 
difficult  than  it  is  in  actual  practice  to  isolate  the  particular  scale 
we  may  be  interested  in.  But  by  and  large,  because  of  the  rela- 
tivity of  values  or  norms  in  a  given  milieu  at  a  given  time,  it  is 
feasible  to  construct  scales  that  deal,  for  example,  with  attitudes 
toward  different  races,  government  regulations,  religion,  occupa- 
tional prestige.  Likewise,  everyday  life  judgments  such  as  pre- 
dictions made  concerning  the  outcome  of  social  conflicts  are  gen- 
erally found  to  be  made  within  a  limited  scalable  range.  [83] 

We  have  already  mentioned  the  fact  that  attitudes  concerned 
with  a  person's  position  or  status  in  social  life  are  ego-involved  and 
that  such  ego-involved  experiences  have  affective  properties,  (p.  21) 
Since  many  attitudes  related  to  social  situations  are  more  or  less 
enduring  affective  fixations,  they  acquire  a  special  importance  to 
the  individual.  They  become  major  constituents  of  the  ego.  The 
relationship  between  these  social  attitudes  and  the  ego  structure 
and  the  bearing  of  this  relationship  to  motivation  are  considered 
in  the  next  chapter.  The  significant  fact  that  most  social  attitudes 
are  ego-involved  should,  however,  be  borne  in  mind  in  connection 
with  the  material  reported  here. 

In  his  series  of  experiments  (one  of  which  was  referred  to  in  the 
last  chapter)  Coffin  investigated  the  psychology  of  suggestion  as 
it  was  related  both  to  attitudinal  structure  and  to  stimulus  situa- 
tions. [31]  In  his  first  experiment  Coffin  studied  the  relationship 
between  an  individual's  attitude  and  the  type  of  propaganda  to 
which  he  was  particularly  susceptible.  He  found  (in  the  winter 
of  1939  and  1940)  significant  correlations  between  pro-Allied  atti- 
tudes on  the  one  hand  and  the  acceptance  of  specially  prepared 
pro-Allied  propaganda  on  the  other  hand.  Conversely,  those  with 
pro-German  attitudes  accepted  pro-German  propaganda  to  a  sig- 
nificant degree.  In  a  second  experiment  Coffin  used  the  Rorschach 
ink  blots  as  stimuli  with  little  structuration.  After  his  subjects 
had  rank-ordered  a  list  of  ten  occupations  according  to  their  so- 


62      ATTITUDE  FORMULATION  EXTENDED  TO  CONCRETE  SITUATIONS 

cial  standing,  they  were  divided  into  two  groups;  each  group  was 
told  the  characteristic  response  given  to  the  Rorschach  ink  blots 
by  professional  men,  by  business  men,  by  skilled  laborers,  and  by 
those  on  WPA,  but  the  various  characteristics  were  attributed  to 
different  occupations  in  the  two  experimental  groups.  The  results 
clearly  showed  that,  when  these  ambiguous  stimuli  were  used,  the 
subjects  were  highly  influenced  by  the  suggestions  given  which 
served  as  anchorages.  They  actively  structured  the  imaginative 
situation  according  to  the  "characteristic  reaction"  to  the  blots  of 
occupational  groups  they  believed  had  high  social  standing.  In 
a  third  experiment,  testing  the  relationship  between  suggestibility 
and  the  difficulty  of  problem  solution,  Coffin  found  that,  when 
mathematical  problems  were  arranged  in  order  of  difficulty  with 
marginal  (and  usually  false)  hints  (anchorages)  beside  each  prob- 
lem as  to  what  procedure  might  be  used  in  its  solution,  respondents 
more  highly  trained  in  mathematics  followed  the  marginal  sug- 
gestions less:  those  who  knew  the  most  mathematics  accepted  less 
than  half  as  many  of  the  suggestions  as  those  who  knew  least 
mathematics.  Those  persons,  therefore,  least  able  to  make  suc- 
cessful checks  for  themselves  were  most  susceptible  to  an  imposed 
instruction. 

We  have  cited  Coffin's  experiments  here  at  some  length,  since 
they  provide  unequivocal  evidence  of  two  important  conclusions: 
(1)  If  a  suggested  stimulus  is  related  to  and  consistent  with  an 
established  frame  of  reference,  it  is  likely  to  be  accepted;  and,  (2) 
if  a  stimulus  situation  is  ambiguous  or  relatively  meaningless  be- 
cause of  its  difficulty,  then  a  frame  of  reference  verbally  imposed 
by  the  experimenter  is  readily  accepted  as  the  basis  for  judgment. 

A  study  by  Ruth  and  Eugene  Hartley  concerned  with  poorly 
defined  situations  is  particularly  significant  since  it  shows  how  in- 
dividuals establish  "individually  characteristic  ranges  and  refer- 
ence points"  by  means  of  which  they  judge  a  relatively  unstruc- 
tured social  stimulus.  [43]  The  Hartleys  had  students  rate  a  series 
of  pictures  of  completely  unknown  men.  Among  other  ratings, 
the  subjects  were  asked  to  estimate  each  man  pictured  on  his  "gen- 
eral ability"  and  his  "likeliness  to  succeed."  They  found  a  con- 
stancy of  ranges  and  central  tendencies  in  the  judgments  and 
conclude  that 


ATTITUDE  FORMULATION  EXTENDED  TO  CONCRETE  SITUATIONS      63 

In  making  primary  evaluative  social  responses  to  people,  when  little 
information  is  available,  individuals  tend  to  manifest  characteristic 
ranges  and  central  tendencies  with  reference  to  which  judgments  are 
made.  [43] 

The  research  of  McGarvey  "was  undertaken  in  order  to  deter- 
mine whether  relations  similar  to  those  which  have  been  found 
to  hold  with  the  psychophysical  materials  would  appear  also  in 
judgments  of  verbal  materials  along  value-dimensions."  [66,  26] 
In  her  first  experiment,  McGarvey  had  subjects  rate  the  social 
prestige  attached  to  a  number  of  occupations.  After  they  had  rated 
all  occupations  on  a  graphic  scale,  in  further  experimental  sessions 
the  top  or  the  bottom  category  of  the  scale  was  set  by  an  "anchor- 
ing value."  In  the  second  experiment  the  items  judged  were  short 
descriptions  of  various  types  of  social  behavior  to  be  evaluated  in 
terms  of  their  desirability.  Again  "anchoring  stimuli"  were  in- 
troduced at  either  end  of  the  scale.  On  the  basis  of  these  experi- 
ments McGarvey  concluded  that 

...  the  effect  of  the  anchoring  value  was  that  of  bringing  about  an 
extension  of  the  absolute  scale  upward  or  downward  in  the  direction 
of  the  anchoring  value.  The  extension  of  the  absolute  scale  involves 
not  only  a  displacement  of  the  scale  with  reference  to  the  range  of 
values  represented  by  the  stimulus-series,  but  also  a  widening  of  the 
categories  of  response, — a  finding  in  complete  agreement  with  the 
results  obtained  with  psychophysical  material.  [66,  78] 

In  other  words,  judgment  of  a  given  stimulus  is  found  to  be  deter- 
mined by  the  frame  of  reference  within  which  that  judgment  oc- 
curred, and  the  introduction  of  new  anchorages  changed  the 
dimensions  of  reference  frames.  This  precisely  parallels  on  a  social 
level  the  results  obtained  by  Rogers  reported  in  the  last  chapter. 

The  close  relationship  between  memory  and  an  individual's 
frame  of  reference  was  exhaustively  shown  by  Bartlett  who  con- 
cluded that  "remembering  is  'schematically'  determined,"  [8,  312] 
"an  imaginative  reconstruction,  or  construction,  built  out  of  the 
relation  of  our  attitude  towards  a  whole  active  mass  of  organized 
past  reactions  or  experience,  and  to  a  little  outstanding  detail  which 
commonly  appears  in  image  or  in  language  form."  [213] 


64     ATTITUDE  FORMULATION  EXTENDED  TO  CONCRETE  SITUATIONS 

The  circumstances  that  arouse  memory  orientations,  whether  they 
occur  in  the  laboratory  or  in  everyday  life,  always  set  up  an  attitude 
that  is  primarily  towards  a  particular  "schematic"  organization.  [8, 312] 

What  a  person  remembers,  as  well  as  what  a  person  observes  of  a 
given  stimulus  situation,  was,  Bartlett  found,  clearly  influenced  by 
the  particular  social  origin  of  the  individual's  attitude.  He  showed 
how  these  attitudes,  and  consequently  recall  and  observation,  varied 
according  to  the  cultural  background  of  the  individual,  how  they 
differed  among  members  of  various  groups  within  a  given  social 
system,  and  how  they  differed  within  the  same  individual  when 
he  was  or  was  not  in  the  actual  social  presence  of  other  members  of 
his  group. 

Various  subsequent  studies  have  indicated  that  material  consist- 
ent with  a  person's  attitude  is  much  more  likely  to  be  remembered 
than  material  not  consistent  with  it.  Seeleman,  in  stating  the 
problem  of  her  experimental  study  of  memory,  says  that  "these 
differences  in  standards  may  be  expressed  as  differences  in  subjec- 
tive norms  or  frames  of  reference/'  [77,  7]  She  analyzed  the  in- 
fluence of  the  attitude  toward  the  Negro  on  the  remembrance  of 
pictures  of  whites  and  Negroes  to  which  were  attached,  in  one 
experiment,  favorable  or  unfavorable  phrases  supposedly  describ- 
ing the  individual  shown.  She  found  that  persons  with  extremely 
unfavorable  attitudes  toward  the  Negro  recognized  fewer  indi- 
vidual differences  between  the  Negroes  shown  in  the  pictures, 
recognized  correctly  fewer  Negro  than  white  pictures,  assigned  to 
Negro  pictures  more  unfavorable  phrases  and  remembered  these 
unfavorable  phrases  attached  to  Negro  pictures  more  accurately 
than  did  persons  favorable  to  Negroes.  The  evidence  from  these 
experiments  is,  then,  that  the  attitude  toward  the  Negro  affects 
perception  as  well  as  memory. 

Wood  [93]  studied  the  changes  occurring  when  subjects  with 
favorable  and  unfavorable  attitudes  toward  Negroes  wrote  ab- 
stracts of  an  article  concerning  differences  between  Negroes  and 
whites.  She  found  that  subjects  omitted  and  distorted  statements 
from  the  article  and  added  new  statements  in  the  direction  of  their 
own  attitudes.  When  these  somewhat  biased  and  ambiguous  ab- 
stracts were  read  and  restated  by  other  subjects  with  similar  atti- 


ATTITUDE  FORMULATION  EXTENDED  TO  CONCRETE  SITUATIONS      65 

tudes,  the  extent  of  change  in  the  direction  of  the  subjects'  atti- 
tudes was  even  greater  than  when  the  original,  more  structured, 
and  objective  article  was  abstracted. 

G.  W.  Allport  and  Kramer  have  reported  a  study  in  which  they 
found  that  persons  who  were  most  anti-Semitic  were  the  most 
accurate  judges  of  Semitism  from  pictures,  [4]  Zillig  showed  ex- 
perimentally that  women  tend  to  remember  more  items  favorable 
to  women,  whereas  men  tend  to  remember  more  items  favorable 
to  men.  [95]  Watson  and  Hartmann  demonstrated  that  a  person's 
attitude  toward  atheism  clearly  determined  what  he  would  re- 
member of  material  concerning  atheism  and  theism.  [90]  Ed- 
wards discovered  that  the  political  attitudes  of  his  subjects  signifi- 
cantly determined  their  recognition  of  items  contained  in  a  speech 
they  had  previously  heard  about  the  New  Deal.  [34]  In  another 
study  Edwards  demonstrated  the  extent  to  which  a  person's  atti- 
tude stimulates  him  to  rationalize  his  answers  to  factual  statements 
with  which  he  disagrees.  [35]  Levine  and  Murphy  have  shown 
experimentally  that  both  the  learning  and  the  forgetting  of  pas- 
sages favorable  or  unfavorable  to  the  Soviet  Union  are  significantly 
affected  by  a  person's  attitude  toward  Communism.  [63] 

The  psychology  of  testimony  presents  many  vivid  illustrations  of 
the  influence  of  attitude  on  observation,  judgment,  and  memory. 
Stern's  early  experiments  on  Aussage  showed  the  suggestive  effect 
of  leading  questions  and  demonstrated  that  the  effect  of  such  sug- 
gestions often  became  "stabilized"  to  influence  later  or  allied  judg- 
ments. He  also  demonstrated  how  persons  tended  to  describe 
occurrences  from  the  point  of  view  of  what  was  to  them  the  "cus- 
tomary" or  "usual"  way  for  such  occurrences  to  take  place.  The 
attitude  a  witness  has  toward  a  dispute  and  a  witness'  own  attitudes 
toward  situations  were  apt  to  have  considerable  influence  on  the 
testimony  of  the  witness,  even  though  he  might  be  a  highly  edu- 
cated cautious  individual.  [86,  87] 

In  an  unpublished  study  by  Lazarsfeld  and  associates  of  the  re- 
action of  individuals  of  various  nationality  and  racial  backgrounds 
to  the  same  motion  picture,  the  effect  of  attitudes  in  determining 
what  will  or  will  not  be  observed  and  remembered  and  what  emo- 
tional reactions  will  be  aroused  in  different  people  by  the  same 
stimulus  situation  is  clearly  demonstrated.  The  film  was  a  short 


66      ATTITUDE  FORMULATION  EXTENDED  TO  CONCRETE  SITUATIONS 

British  war  picture,  Naples  Is  a  Battleground,  shown  to  experimen- 
tal groups  in  the  early  summer  of  1944.  Native  white  Americans 
tended  to  see  in  the  film  an  example  of  American  armed  might 
and  remembered  particularly  the  showing  of  General  Mark  Clark; 
Negroes  in  the  audience  paid  special  attention  to  the  Negro  troops 
pictured;  while  most  marked  of  all  were  the  reactions  of  first- 
generation  Italians  who  were  impressed,  saddened,  or  horrified  by 
the  ruins  and  extent  of  devastation.  The  experimenters  observed 
that 

There  were  certain  aspects  of  this  film  that  stood  out  for  some  but 
not  for  others  because  of  their  differing  mental  sets  as  they  watched 
it.  [61] 

A  study  of  rumors  made  by  Knapp  in  1942  showed  that  "rumors 
become  harmonized  with  the  cultural  traditions  of  the  group  in 
which  they  circulate."  [57,  30]  Rumors  are  given  particular  twists 
as  they  penetrate  groups  where  characteristic  norms  are  found, 
such  as  a  high  degree  of  anti-Semitism,  anti-Negro  and  anti- 
British  sentiment.  He  noted,  for  example,  that  in  the  Italian  sec- 
tion of  Boston  rumors  of  enemy  submarines  outside  Boston  Harbor 
were  similar  to  rumors  in  other  areas  of  the  city  except  that  people 
in  the  Italian  section  believed  the  submarines  were  Italian. 

An  experiment  by  George  H.  Smith  [82]  comparing  the  effects 
of  fact  and  rumor  labels  on  the  credibility  of  news  items  has  spe- 
cial relevance  for  our  problem.  Smith  prepared  as  "news  items" 
26  statements  concerning  the  Soviet  Union.  Half  the  statements 
were  favorable  to  the  Soviet  Union,  half  unfavorable.  One  third 
of  all  subjects  were  given  the  statements  as  "actual  facts"  which 
had  been  completely  checked,  one  third  received  them  as  "unveri- 
fied rumors"  for  which  the  experimenter  had  failed  to  find  factual 
support,  while  the  remaining  third  reacted  to  statements  that  had 
no  label  attached.  In  addition,  all  subjects  checked  a  scale  measur- 
ing their  attitudes  toward  the  Soviet  Union. 

Smith  found  that 

The  absolute  amount  of  belief  an  individual  places  in  the  "news" 
items,  under  any  condition  of  labeling,  almost  certainly  depends  in 
part  on  the  initial  attitude  with  which  he  approaches  the  items  .  .  . 


ATTITUDE  FORMULATION  EXTENDED  TO  CONCRETE  SITUATIONS      67 

the  plausibility  of  statements  seems  to  be  judged  on  the  basis  of  some 
established  scale.  [82] 

When  a  statement  was  labeled  a  "fact,"  judgment  was  considerably 
more  altered  than  when  it  was  labeled  a  "rumor,"  although  the 
"rumor"  label  did  have  some  effect. 

The  fact  label  swings  the  subjects  in  the  direction  of  greater  belief 
and  .  .  .  the  rumor  label  edges  them  toward  lesser  belief,  as  compared 
with  no  label  at  all.  [82] 

Smith's  interpretation  is  that 

The  "fact"  label  provides  a  tenuous  standard  of  judgment  which 
helps  people  to  interpret  a  relatively  ambiguous  situation;  but  its 
effectiveness  varies  with  its  consistency  to  objective  criteria  or  to 
established  standards  of  judgment.  ...  It  [the  "fact"  label]  probably 
served  as  a  point  of  reference  in  "critical  moments"  when  other  factors 
were  not  adequate  to  lead  to  a  decision.  [82] 

On  the  other  hand, 

The  rumor  label  is  similar  to  no  label  at  all  in  that  it  constitutes  an 
ambiguous  stimulus  which  forces  people  to  interpret,  or  structure,  the 
situation  for  themselves.  [82] 

Numerous  experiments  on  the  level  of  aspiration  have  shown 
how  the  same  performance  can  be  regarded  either  as  a  success  or 
a  failure,  depending  on  the  frame  of  reference  in  which  the  per- 
formance occurs,  (pp.  120  ff.)  In  the  work  of  Chapman  and 
Volkmann  previously  mentioned,  the  setting  of  a  level  of  aspiration 
was  studied  under  two  sets  of  conditions.  [30]  In  one  experiment 
involving  familiarity  with  the  authors  of  various  literary  passages, 
each  of  the  different  experimental  groups  was  given  in  advance 
of  their  direct  acquaintance  with  the  task  involved  the  score  sup- 
posedly made  by  another  group.  The  score  attributed  to  each 
group  was  the  same  but  the  groups  varied  in  their  prestige:  authors 
and  literary  critics,  students  similar  to  the  subjects,  and  WPA 
workers.  Results  here  show  a  clear-cut  tendency  for  those  com- 
paring themselves  to  a  superior  group  to  lower  their  aspiration 
level,  those  comparing  themselves  to  an  inferior  group  to  raise 
their  aspiration  level.  In  their  second  experiment,  involving  a  test 
of  mental  ability,  all  subjects  were  given  a  test  in  two  sessions  be- 


68      ATTITUDE  FORMULATION  EXTENDED  TO  CONCRETE  SITUATIONS 

fore  any  attempt  was  made  to  change  their  aspiration  level  by  com- 
paring their  performance  to  inferior  or  superior  groups.  Under 
these  conditions,  no  significant  change  in  the  level  of  aspiration 
was  produced;  "the  subjects'  own  previous  scores  provided  the 
most  effective  anchoring."  [235]  In  the  conditions  of  the  first 
experiment,  the  stimulus  situation  is  unstructured,  and  an  imposed 
norm  is  accepted  as  a  frame  of  reference;  in  the  conditions  of  the 
second  experiment,  the  stimulus  situation  has  become  structured 
through  experience,  and  imposed  norms  are  ineffectual. 

Asch,  Block,  and  Hertzman  showed  that  judgments  concern- 
ing the  characteristics  of  different  professional  groups  tend  to  be 
considerably  modified  when  an  evaluation  from  an  authoritative 
source  is  introduced.  [6]  A  close  relationship  was  found  between 
judgments  on  different  characteristics  of  various  professions,  indi- 
cating that  most  of  the  judgments  seemed  to  derive  from  under- 
lying attitudes.  They  conclude  that  "the  judgments  of  a  single 
situation  are  related  to  each  other  by  a  person  in  accordance  with 
an  underlying  attitude  of  acceptance  or  rejection"  [248],  that  "a 
standard  having  an  authoritative  source  tends  to  alter  an  individ- 
ual's judgments  in  its  direction"  [249],  that  relationship  in  judg- 
ment becomes  most  clearly  established  "for  situations  which  are 
not  well-defined  objectively"  [257],  and  that  "the  observer,  in  the 
absence  of  objective  criteria,  and  in  the  face  of  the  necessity  of 
reaching  some  conclusions,  proceeds  to  arrange  a  scale  of  prefer- 
ence in  terms  of  some  generally  favorable  or  unfavorable  impres- 
sion." [229] 

Kay's  study  of  the  relationship  between  personal  frames  of  refer- 
ence and  social  judgments  indicates  that  an  individual's  evaluation 
of  various  occupations  is  considerably  more  affected  by  accepted 
social  norms  relating  to  those  occupations  than  by  an  individual's 
own  preference  for  an  occupation  or  his  experience  with  it.  [54] 
In  other  words,  the  social  norms  concerning  the  value  and  charac- 
teristics of  common  occupations  are  rather  uncritically  taken  over 
by  individuals  as  personal  frames  of  reference  by  means  of  which 
specific  judgments  are  made.  Analyzing  her  data  further  accord- 
ing to  the  source  of  information  concerning  the  various  occupa- 
tions rated,  Kay  found  that  for  a  third  of  the  occupations  "cultural" 
sources  were  mentioned  most  frequently,  whereas  the  "personal** 


ATTITUDE  FORMULATION  EXTENDED  TO  CONCRETE  SITUATIONS      69 

sources  uncovered  in  the  interviews  were  interpreted  or  reacted  to 
on  the  basis  of  "personal  frames  of  reference"  and  "in  the  light 
of  existing  social  norms"  rather  than  on  the  basis  of  the  "objective 
quality  of  the  experiences  themselves."  [55,  363]  This  interpreta- 
tion confirms  Davis'  study  on  the  attitudes  of  children  in  Soviet 
Russia  with  its  finding  that  Russian  children  rated  laboring  people 
high  and  lawyers  and  bankers  low.  [32]  In  their  examination  of 
the  basis  of  prestige  judgments  of  various  occupational  groups, 
Osgood  and  Stagner  demonstrated  that  the  "decisions  about  char- 
acteristics of  occupational  stereotypes  tend  to  conform  closely  to 
a  framework  which  is  based  on  the  relative  prestige  of  occupa- 
tions." [75,  287]  They  conclude  that 

...  the  mere  presentation  of  a  set  of  occupational  stereotypes  for  a 
series  of  judgments  caused  our  subjects  spontaneously  to  establish  a 
prestige  framework  which  then  determined  in  a  highly  reliable  man- 
ner judgments  on  the  specific  traits  listed.  [75,  289] 

Asch  found  that  an  individual's  judgment  of  relatively  ill-defined 
and  unclear  situations  could  be  changed  when  the  imputed  judg- 
ment of  congenial  groups  was  introduced  as  a  reference.  [7]  On 
the  other  hand,  subjects  tended  to  reject  the  judgment  of  antago- 
nistic groups.  Here  we  have  evidence  that  an  individual  tends  to 
accept  an  imposed  norm  as  his  own  frame  of  reference  for  judg- 
ing a  situation  when  that  situation  is  itself  unclear  or  when  he  has 
no  pre-existing,  sure,  or  ego-involved  frame  of  reference  of  his  own. 
An  analysis  of  radio  listeners  who  followed  a  particular  commen- 
tator indicated  that  the  chief  function  of  a  news  commentator  is 
to  provide  frames  of  reference  by  means  of  which  listeners  can 
judge  the  plethora  of  events  going  on  around  them.  [22]  The  less 
people  know  about  objective  conditions,  die  more  they  depend  on 
a  commentator  to  tell  them  what  these  events  mean  and  to  help 
them  select  items  to  read  in  their  newspapers. 

One  of  the  most  penetrating  studies  of  the  development  of  preva- 
lent attitudes  in  our  social  system  is  that  of  Horowitz  on  the 
genesis  of  attitudes  toward  the  Negro.  [45]  He  demonstrated  un- 
equivocally that  this  attitude  is  imposed  bodily  and  uncritically 
without  any  basis  in  experience  or  knowledge.  He  concludes  "that 
attitudes  toward  Negroes  are  now  chiefly  determined  not  by  con- 


70      ATTITUDE  FORMULATION  EXTENDED  TO  CONCRETE  SITUATIONS 

tact  with  Negroes,  but  by  contact  with  the  prevalent  attitude  to- 
ward Negroes."  [35]  Horowitz  developed  a  series  of  ingenious 
tests  to  measure  attitudes  toward  the  Negro  objectively.  These 
tests  were  administered  to  children  and  adolescents  in  a  variety  of 
social  groups — children  in  the  rural  and  urban  South,  all  white 
groups  in  New  York  City,  mixed  groups  in  New  York  City,  and 
a  group  of  New  York  Communist  children.  His  tests  showed 
that  children  in  New  York  City  were  just  as  prejudiced  as  the 
children  in  the  South,  that  children  in  mixed  schools  were  as 
prejudiced  as  those  in  all  white  schools,  that  contact  with  popular 
Negro  children  had  no  effect  on  attitudes,  and  that  the  only  group 
of  children  tested  who  had  no  prejudice  against  the  Negro  were 
children  of  Communist  parents — people  devoid  of  racial  prejudices 
which  they  would  pass  on  to  their  children.  Furthermore,  Horo- 
witz found  that  Negro  boys  in  mixed  schools  tended  to  accept 
some  of  the  racial  attitudes  of  the  white  majority  of  their  group.2 
His  findings  confirm  the  earlier  conclusions  of  Lasker's  well- 
known  study  that  race  attitudes  in  children  are  due  chiefly  to  "the 
absorption  of  adult  attitudes,"  [60,  371]  and  that  contacts  between 
children  of  different  races  are  almost  invariably  influenced  by  the 
"adult-made  environment."  [371]  G.  W.  Allport  and  Kramer 
found  that  if  this  adult  environment  contains  institutionalized  re- 
ligious training  a  young  person  is  more  likely  to  be  prejudiced 
against  minority  groups  than  if  such  institutionalized  religious 
training  is  absent.  [4]  Such  findings  as  these  indicate  once  more 
that,  when  rigidly  established  norms  are  accepted  by  an  individual, 
his  thinking  on  and  reaction  to  related  situations  is  highly  in- 
flexible. [65] 

In  a  significant  study  concerned  with  the  development  of  stereo- 
types toward  the  Negro,  Blake  and  Dennis  [12]  had  students  from 
grades  4  through  11  in  a  southern  school  compare  whites  and  Ne- 
groes on  60  characteristics.  Their  results  showed  that  there  was 
less  agreement  among  the  younger  children  who  had  a  "relatively 
undifferentiated"  attitude  unfavorable  to  the  Negro.  They  con- 
clude that 

2  This  is  reminiscent  of  the  fact  that  in  the  days  of  slavery  in  the  United  States 
there  was  a  sizable  number  of  freed  Negro  slaves  who,  when  they  acquired  land 
of  their  own,  retained  Negro  slaves  to  work  it  [39] 


ATTITUDE  FORMULATION  EXTENDED  TO  CONCRETE  SITUATIONS      71 

.  .  .  the  young  white  child  acquires  first  of  all  a  generally  unfavor- 
able attitude  toward  the  Negro,  which  makes  him  unwilling  to  at- 
tribute to  the  Negro  any  "good"  traits.  With  increased  age  and 
experience,  the  child  gradually  learns  to  apply  the  adult  stereotypes,  a 
few  of  which  are  complimentary.  [12,  531] 

The  extent  to  which  the  accepted  attitude  toward  the  Negro  affects 
judgment  and  goes  against  all  evidence  has  been  illustrated  by 
a  public  opinion  survey  made  in  1944  on  a  nationwide  sample  of 
the  adult  white  population  in  the  United  States.  A  majority  of 
those  with  opinions  believe  that  Negroes  are  not  as  intelligent  as 
white  people  and  cannot  learn  as  well  if  they  are  given  the  same 
education.  And  a  third  of  our  white  population  believes  that 
Negro  blood  somehow  differs  from  white  blood,  a  third  of  the 
people  believe  it  is  the  same,  and  a  third  say  they  don't  know.  [72] 
Murphy  and  Likert  concluded  from  their  study  of  attitudes  to- 
ward minority  groups  that 

.  .  .  the  individual  usually  acquires  his  prejudices  against  a  minor- 
ity group  not  primarily  from  contact  with  this  minority  group  but 
chiefly  from  contact  with  the  prevailing  attitude  toward  this  minority 
group.  [70,  136] 

The  acceptance  of  attitudes  is  further  confirmed  by  studies  which 
have  shown  the  ease  with  which  people  characterize  various  na- 
tionality groups,  irrespective  of  their  lack  of  knowledge  about  or 
experience  with  these  groups.  Katz  and  Braly  found  that  the 
preferential  ranking  of  students  for  various  nationality  and  racial 
groups  closely  followed  a  weighted  ranking  of  the  judgment  of  a 
comparable  group  of  students  on  the  stereotype  of  these  same 
groups.  [52]  Public  opinion  surveys  have  shown  that  less  than 
10  per  cent  of  the  American  population  feel  unable  to  select  from 
a  list  of  adjectives  those  that  best  describe  various  nationality  groups 
and  that  people  in  our  culture  have  generally  similar  stereotypes 
concerning  the  characteristics  of  major  nationality  and  racial 
groups.  [15] 

Particularly  significant  as  an  indication  of  the  way  in  which 
even  minority  groups  accept  the  values  of  the  larger  macrocosm 
is  the  comparison  that  can  be  made  between  the  characterizations 
of  various  racial  groups  by  white  and  Negro  Americans.  For 


72      ATTITUDE  FORMULATION  EXTENDED  TO  CONCRETE  SITUATIONS 

example,  Meenes's  results  [68]  on  the  Katz-Braly  list  of  racial 
characteristics  when  used  with  Negro  students  are  by  and  large 
consistent  with  those  obtained  on  white  students  in  1933.  [51] 
White  students  at  Princeton  and  Negro  students  at  Howard  had 
essentially  similar  stereotypes  concerning  such  peoples  as  the  Jews, 
English,  Irish,  Turks.  This  is  quite  in  keeping  with  Horowitz' 
finding  previously  mentioned  concerning  the  acceptance  of  white 
attitudes  by  Negro  children  in  mixed  schools.  The  single  notable 
exception  in  Meenes's  study  is  the  tendency  of  the  Negro  students 
to  judge  their  own  racial  group  on  the  basis  of  a  different  frame 
and  hence  to  assign  to  it  fewer  uncomplimentary  adjectives,  such 
as  "lazy"  or  "ignorant." 

The  extent  to  which  stereotypes  influence  judgment  has  been 
clearly  demonstrated  experimentally.  Sherif,  for  example,  showed 
that  college  students  both  in  the  United  States  and  in  Turkey  were 
significantly  affected  in  their  rating  of  a  literary  passage  by  the 
name  of  the  author  attributed  to  that  passage.  [80]  The  passages 
used  in  the  United  States  were  all  by  one  author,  those  used  in 
Turkey  were  all  by  another  author.  The  judgments  of  these  pas- 
sages, then,  were  made  largely  in  terms  of  established  values. 
These  results  are  similar  to  earlier  demonstrations  of  everyday 
stereotypes  shown  by  Zillig  [95]  among  others  and  were  confirmed 
later  on  still  another  type  of  material,  [19] 

It  is  obvious  that  the  whole  psychology  of  fads  and  fashions  is 
to  be  explained  largely  in  terms  of  accepted  norms  and  values  that 
provide  standards  of  judgment  for  style,  correctness,  and  certain 
criteria  for  status,  beauty,  significance,  and  the  like  (pp.  348#.). 
In  a  note  "concerning  scientific  progress"  at  the  end  of  his  Sensa- 
tion and  Perception  in  the  History  of  Experimental  Psychology, 
Boring  shows  that  even  scientists,  including  psychologists,  tend  to 
conform  to  the  Zeitgeist  and  are  retarded  by  habits  of  thought 
currently  fashionable  or  by  "laboratory  atmosphere."  [13]  It 
should  be  pointed  out  in  this  connection,  however,  that  atmos- 
pheres of  psychological  laboratories,  as  well  as  more  common  fads 
and  fashions,  sooner  or  later  change  with  new  factors  imposed  by 
objective  conditions  or  by  the  accumulation  of  evidence.  The 
dresses  women  have  for  everyday  wear  in  the  western  world  can 
no  longer  be  styled  without  reference  to  the  demands  of  the  ma- 


ATTITUDE  FORMULATION  EXTENDED  TO  CONCRETE  SITUATIONS      73 

chine  age  in  which  they  live;  the  Titchenerian  influence  could  not 
remain  unaffected  by  the  results  of  Wiirzburg.  As  facts  pile  up  in 
psychology  or  any  other  science,  laboratory  atmospheres  and 
"schools  of  thought"  become  increasingly  tenuous. 

Newcomb  measured  the  change  of  attitude*  of  students  in  a 
small  college  community  where  nonconservative  attitudes  were 
considered  to  be  more  "proper"  and  to  carry  more  prestige  than 
conservative  attitudes.  [71]  He  found  that  this  "community  frame 
of  reference"  [151]  significantly  influenced  the  attitudes  of  students 
in  the  liberal  direction  during  their  four  years,  irrespective  of  the 
courses  studied  in  college.  Newcomb  describes  this  shift  of  atti- 
tude as  general  rather  than  as  a  shift  of  a  series  of  specific  atti- 
tudes toward  specific  issues.  When  events  or  new  proposals  were 
reacted  to,  the  more  advanced  students  more  consistently  reacted  to 
them  in  a  liberal  way. 

The  way  in  which  an  individual's  own  present  income  provides 
a  reference  point  for  judging  financial  needs  and  aspiration  has 
been  demonstrated  by  Centers  and  Cantril.  [29]  In  a  nationwide 
survey  concerned  with  the  relationship  of  present  income  to  satis- 
faction and  wants,  they  found  that,  among  those  people  dissatis- 
fied with  their  present  income,  the  larger  the  income  is,  the  more 
additional  money  is  wanted.  An  analysis  of  judgments  made  by 
persons  in  different  income  groups  of  the  income  tax  people  in 
various  income  brackets  should  pay  shows  that  those  in  the  low 
income  groups  have  such  inadequate  standards  for  judging  the 
incomes  of  people  in  high  income  brackets  that  they  tend  to  find 
such  judgments  difficult  or  impossible  to  make.  Furthermore, 
when  judgments  are  made  by  persons  in  the  lower  income  groups 
of  the  tax  which  those  in  the  highest  brackets  should  pay,  the 
figure  given  is  significantly  less  than  the  tax  upper  income  groups 
think  should  be  paid  by  the  rich,  illustrating  again  the  way  in 
which  judgment  is  anchored  in  individual  frames  of  reference.  [40] 

From  his  investigation  of  the  effect  of  the  monetary  value  of  ob- 
jects on  the  perception  of  number,  Ansbacher  [5]  whose  research 
follows  Brunswik's  [17]  concluded  that  "monetary  value  through 
familiarity  does  influence  perception  under  certain  circumstances." 
[56]  Ansbacher  used  stamps  of  different  denominations  in  his 
experiment.  To  study  the  variable  of  familiarity,  both  Canadian 


74      ATTITUDE  FORMULATION  EXTENDED  TO  CONCRETE  SITUATIONS 

and  American  stamps  were  shown  to  Canadian  and  American 
observers.  Subjects  were  asked  to  compare  the  number  of  stamps 
of  one  denomination  in  one  group  of  stamps  with  the  number  of 
another  denomination  in  another  group.  Under  other  instructions 
comparisons  were  made  of  the  value  represented  by  the  two  groups 
of  stamps.  A  controlled  psychophysical  series,  using  stamps  of  the 
same  denomination,  was  introduced  in  the  experimental  sessions. 
Ansbacher  found  that  the  judgment  of  the  number  of  stamps  of 
different  denominations  "is  more  variable  than  a  comparable  psy- 
chophysical judgment,  viz.,  where  both  standard  and  variable 
groups  consist  of  the  same  stamps."  [83]  A  constant  error  ap- 
peared, similar  to  that  found  on  judgments  of  size. 

In  familiar  stamps  we  find  after  psychophysical  judgments  a  slight 
tendency  to  underestimate  the  more  valuable  group  with  respect  to 
number  [83]  .  .  .  value  through  familiarity  does  affect  perception. 
[84] 

Especially  significant  for  us  is  Ansbacher 's  finding  that  the  greater 
variability  in  the  number  judgments  disappears  "if  the  number 
judgment  is  preceded  by  a  psychophysical  judgment  .  .  .  preced- 
ing psychophysical  judgments  seem  to  strengthen  the  number  as- 
pect at  the  expense  of  all  incidental  objects."  [83]  In  other  words, 
if  the  subjects  are  able  to  establish  a  scale  by  repeated  exposure  to 
the  series  of  stimuli,  that  scale,  rather  than  one  influenced  by  an 
imposed  norm  is  used  for  reference. 

In  a  series  of  interesting  experiments  where  coins  were  used  as 
stimuli,  Bruner  and  associates  [1,  42]  have  recently  studied  "the 
influence  of  social  value  and  social  need  upon  the  perception  of 
objects  under  ambiguous  and  unambiguous  conditions."  [16,  241] 
They  investigated  the  hypotheses  that 

...  (a)  the  greater  the  social  value  of  an  object,  the  more  will  it 
undergo  perceptual  distortion  away  from  "objective"  size,  color, 
weight,  etc.,  (b)  the  stronger  the  individual  need  for  such  valued  ob- 
jects, the  greater  will  be  the  distorting  effect  of  value,  (c)  ambiguity 
in  the  perceptual  field  will  either  facilitate  or  reduce  value  distortion 
depending  upon  whether  it  favors  or  opposes  the  prominence  of 
value  cues.  [241] 


ATTITUDE  FORMULATION  EXTENDED  TO  CONCRETE  SITUATIONS      75 

In  the  first  experiment,  children  from  two  groups  with  widely 
differing  economic  status  (from  a  private  school  and  a  settlement 
house)  were  asked  to  judge  the  size  of  coins  from  a  penny  to  a 
half  dollar. 

For  both  groups,  the  greater  the  value  of  the  coin,  the  greater  the 
constant  error  of  overestimation.  Pennies  were  seen  as  10  per  cent 
larger  than  actual  size,  half  dollars  35  per  cent  larger.  The  tendency 
of  overestimation  is  significantly  more  marked  among  poor  than 
among  rich  children.  [241] 

When  the  same  experiment  was  repeated  with  adults,  judgments 
continued  to  show  a  distortion  but  one  that  was  more  discrimina- 
tive. The  adults  judged  pennies,  nickels,  and  dimes  as  smaller 
than  their  actual  size;  quarters  and  half-dollars  as  larger.  Further 
experiments  with  adults  using  "made"  money,  showed  that  sym- 
bols with  the  highest  value  (dollars)  produced  greatest  overesti- 
mation, that  a  meaningful  but  not  highly  valued  symbol  (swas- 
tika) produced  less  overestimation,  while  a  meaningless  symbol 
produced  still  less.8  As  the  investigators  point  out,  these  results 
"suggest  principles  going  beyond  Weber's  Law  and  Hollingworth's 
central  tendency  effect."  [241}  And  they  are  entirely  consistent 
with  the  findings  of  Bartlett,  Coffin,  and  others  already  reported. 
Kornhauser's  investigations  of  the  class  attitudes  of  various  socio- 
economic  groups  have  shown  that  in  our  American  social  system 
"logically  opposed  interests"  do  not  by  any  means  lead  to  uni- 
formly opposed  class  attitudes.  [58, 59]  Kornhauser  did  find,  how- 
ever, that  those  persons  within  each  economic  group  who  were 
most  dissatisfied  in  terms  of  their  present  status  and  opportunity 
did  accept  less  frequently  than  others  the  traditional  status  quo  as 
measured  by  attitudes  concerning  labor,  government  control,  and 
the  like.  And  although  the  attitudes  of  income  and  occupational 
groups  were  not  diametrically  opposed,  still  significant  differences 
appeared  between  the  attitudes  of  these  groups  on  a  number  of 
social  and  political  issues.  In  other  words,  objective  conditions  did 
not  fit  currently  accepted  economic  and  social  norms.  Particularly 
significant  for  our  purposes  are  Kornhauser's  reports  of  the  in- 

8  The  results  of  this  second  experiment  were  personally  communicated  to  the 
writers  by  J.  S.  Bruner  in  May  1946. 


76      ATTITUDE  FORMULATION  EXTENDED  TO  CONCRETE  SITUATIONS 

stances  where  no  significant  differences  are  found  between  the 
attitudes  of  different  economic  groups.  These  attitudes,  accepted 
rather  uniformly,  were  those  which  reflected  the  traditional  Ameri- 
can ideology  concerning  individual  opportunity  either  for  the  per- 
son himself  or  for  his  children.  In  chapter  6  we  consider  in  more 
detail  the  current  relationship  among  class  identifications,  status, 
and  attitudes  in  the  United  States. 

That  the  attitudes  toward  labels  describing  different  political 
systems  are  by  and  large  uncritically  accepted  without  any  real 
knowledge  of  the  principles  or  implications  of  those  political  sys- 
tems has  been  established  in  a  number  of  different  studies,  Stag- 
ner  demonstrated,  for  example,  that  people  will  have  a  distinctly 
unfavorable  attitude  toward  Fascism  but  at  the  same  time  will  ac- 
cept certain  fascist  doctrines.  [84]  Menefee  comes  to  the  same 
conclusion  in  his  studies  which  reveal  that  if  the  statement  of  a 
political  or  economic  principle  is  labeled  as  "fascist"  it  will  call 
forth  a  much  more  negative  reaction  than  it  will  without  the  label. 
[69]  Edwards  concludes  from  his  investigations  that 

Some  college  students  have  a  far  greater  degree  of  sympathy  for 
certain  fascist  principles  than  might  be  expected  from  their  otherwise 
antagonistic  reaction  to  the  fascist  label.  [36,  580] 

Significantly  enough,  Edwards  found  that  those  persons  who 
labeled  themselves  politically  as  "independents"  were  considerably 
more  critical  of  statements  of  fascist  principles  than  those  who 
called  themselves  Democrats  or  Republicans.  Katz  and  Cantril 
found  that  the  attitude  toward  Fascism  or  Communism  bore  little 
relationship  to  the  knowledge  of  Fascism  or  Communism  among 
college  students  in  1939.  [53]  Their  study  also  shows  that,  al- 
though there  was  an  overwhelming  rejection  of  the  terms  "Fas- 
cism" and  "Communism,"  the  majority  of  students  felt  at  the  time 
(before  World  War  II  had  brought  the  implications  of  Fascism 
into  clearer  focus)  that  Fascism  was  a  good  thing  for  Germany 
and  Italy  but  would  be  a  bad  thing  for  the  United  States,  whereas 
Communism  was  a  good  thing  for  Russia  but  would  be  a  bad 
thing  for  Germany  and  Italy  and  especially  for  the  United  States. 
In  testing  the  effect  of  the  labels  "Communism,"  "Fascism," 
"liberal,"  and  "reactionary,"  on  passages  that  very  obviously 


ATTITUDE  FORMULATION  EXTENDED  TO  CONCRETE  SITUATIONS      77 

stated  a  point  of  view,  Birch  found  that  "even  a  well-structured 
attitude  is  affected  to  some  degree  by  the  application  of  a  so- 
cially disapproved  label."  [11,  310]  The  fact  that  Birch  was  able 
to  shift  the  degree  of  support  given  a  statement  when  different 
labels  were  applied  to  it  but  was  unsuccessful  in  shifting  support 
from  one  statement  to  another,  only  emphasizes  our  contention 
that  if  a  stimulus  situation  is  in  sharp  contrast  to  a  well-established 
frame  to  which  it  is  easily  related,  no  reversals  in  judgment  should 
be  expected.  "Prestige"  labeling  is  effective  in  determining  a  frame 
in  proportion  to  the  ambiguity  of  the  stimulus  situation.  As  we 
have  said  before,  no  realistic  purpose  is  served  by  trying  to  have 
a  person  see  a  square  as  a  circle.  The  investigations  already  cited 
show  that  broad  symbols  do  tend  to  be  accepted  in  social  life  with- 
out objective  reference  since  objective  reference  is  often  not  com- 
pellingly  introduced  in  the  stimulus  situation.  Those  who  use 
prestige  labeling  for  propaganda  purposes  are,  of  course,  eager  to 
shift  attention  from  any  objective  references  that  might  contradict 
the  frame  they  are  attempting  to  build  up. 

Analyses  of  political  attitudes  have  indicated  clearly  that  for  the 
vast  majority  of  modern  Americans  the  way  in  which  they  vote 
is  determined  chiefly  by  the  political  attitude  they  have  accepted 
rather  than  by  any  analysis  of  different  party  platforms  or  candi- 
dates. Hartmann  has  shown  that  individuals  frequently  accept 
the  stated  principles  of  political  parties  they  strongly  disapprove 
of  so  long  as  they  are  unaware  that  these  principles  have  the  en- 
dorsement of  those  parties.  [44]  Cantril  and  Harding  discovered 
that  over  half  of  the  voting  population  in  the  United  States  were 
completely  unable  to  tell  what  the  differences  were  between  the 
major  political  parties  even  though  the  overwhelming  majority 
of  these  same  people  voted  consistently  for  one  or  the  other  of 
the  major  political  parties.  [25]  Their  analysis  of  the  U.  S.  Con- 
gressional election  of  1942  shows  that  the  overwhelming  propor- 
tion of  the  major  issues  of  the  day  which  might  logically  be 
thought  to  have  some  relationship  to  the  way  a  person  voted  and 
which  were  discussed  during  the  election  campaign  as  partisan 
issues  actually  had  little  or  no  importance  in  determining  vote. 
They  also  showed  how  people  rationalize  their  accepted  attitude 
by  claiming  that  they  usually  vote  for  the  candidate  rather  than 


78     ATTITUDE  FORMULATION  EXTENDED  TO  CONCREffe  SITUATIONS 

for  the  party  that  candidate  represents.  At  least  two  thirds  of 
the  people  who  vote  a  straight  party  ticket  claim  that  they  vote 
for  the  man  rather  than  the  party.  Even  the  minority  of  voters 
who  classify  themselves  as  "independents"  appear,  on  closer  scru- 
tiny, to  be  considerably  less  independent  than  they  claim.  At 
least  half  of  the  "independents"  vote  a  straight  party  ticket  and 
well  over  half  of  the  "independents"  vote  according  to  their 
father's  political  affiliation. 

Lazarsfeld,  Berelson,  and  Gaudet  have  shown  that  a  fairly  re- 
liable index  of  "political  predisposition"  can  be  constructed  for  an 
individual  from  a  knowledge  of  three  social  factors:  religious  affil- 
iation, economic  status,  and  place  of  residence.  [62]  They  found 
that  cross  pressures  of  these  three  social  factors  tended  to  delay 
a  person's  decision  as  to  how  he  would  vote.  Relatively  few  vot- 
ers were  sufficiently  affected  by  political  propaganda  to  change 
their  political  predisposition.  Persons  tend  to  expose  themselves 
mainly  to  the  political  propaganda  of  their  own  party,  but  those 
whose  predisposition  is  toward  one  of  the  major  parties  and  who 
expose  themselves  more  than  others  of  their  persuasion  to  opposi- 
tion propaganda  do  tend  to  vote  more  than  others  for  the  opposi- 
tion party.  The  less  structured  a  person's  political  attitude  is,  be- 
cause of  either  lack  of  interest  or  social  pressures,  the  greater  is 
the  variability  of  voting  behavior  and  the  greater  is  the  influence 
of  propaganda  from  various  media  or  the  effect  of  personal  con- 
tact. 

Breslaw  concluded  from  his  detailed  interviews  concerned  with 
the  development  of  political  attitudes  that  political  attitudes  can 
more  appropriately  be  described  as  an  "orientation"  or  "bias"  than 
a  "point  of  view"  logically  arrived  at.  [14] 

Attitudes  emerge  from  the  particular  social  life  which  happens  to 
surround  the  individual.  [14,  65] 

An  attitude 

...  is  an  end  product  with  no  necessary  relationship  to  the  particu- 
lar components  of  that  stimulation.  An  attitude  is  something  that 
becomes  implanted — as  fear  of  the  dark  becomes  implanted — in  many 
different  ways.  [14,  66] 


ATTITUDE  FORMULATION  EXTENDED  TO  CONCRETE  SITUATIONS      79 

The  consistency  with  which  an  individual's  attitude  determines 
his  reactions  to  situations  to  which  he  relates  the  attitude  has  been 
shown  in  a  number  of  different  studies.  Vetter  found  that  a  per- 
son who  was  radical  or  conservative  tended  to  be  radical  or  con- 
servative in  his  reaction  to  a  wide  variety  of  social,  political,  and 
ethical  situations.  [88]  Katz  and  F.  H.  Allport  noted  the  "con- 
sistency with  which  different  attitudes  seem  to  fit  together  in  their 
respective  patterns."  [50,  48]  Cantril  demonstrated  that  an  atti- 
tude has  a  directive  influence  on  numerous  specific  reactions  to 
which  it  is  related,  that  an  attitude  tends  to  be  enduring  and  con- 
stant even  though  the  situations  which  evoke  it  may  fluctuate,  and 
that  an  attitude  can  be  quite  independent  of  the  particular  experi- 
ences which  may  have  established  it.  [18]  Stagner  found  that 
persons  who  are  intensely  chauvinistic  tend  to  react  consistently 
and  unfavorably  to  a  wide  variety  of  issues  such  as  tariff  reduction, 
labor  unions,  or  government  ownership.  [85] 

In  a  study  of  the  reaction  of  the  American  people  to  World 
War  II,  we  find  that,  while  the  religious  frame  of  Catholics  previ- 
ous to  Pearl  Harbor  affected  their  attitude  toward  U.  S.  interven- 
tion in  the  European  war,  it  did  not  affect  their  attitude  toward 
U.  S.  intervention  in  the  Pacific  war — in  the  latter  case  there  was 
no  conflict  between  religious  and  nationalistic  attitudes  influencing 
the  judgments  of  a  stimulus  situation.  [27]  The  conflict  of  atti- 
tudes in  German-American  and  Italian-American  citizens  result- 
ing from  U.  S.  intervention  in  World  War  II  and  the  effect  of  this 
conflict  in  voting  behavior  is  revealed  by  the  findings  of  Bean, 
Mosteller,  and  Williams  that  these  two  groups  significantly  shifted 
their  vote  away  from  Roosevelt  in  the  1940  election.  [9] 

Analysis  of  data  obtained  from  public  opinion  polls  shows  that 
major  events  are  judged  in  terms  of  frames  of  reference  which 
enable  people  to  relate  these  events  to  their  own  self-interest. 
Hence  public  opinion  on  specific  issues  is  highly  sensitive  to 
events,  [26]  Polls  further  show  that,  once  the  vast  majority  of 
individuals  become  aware  that  a  social,  economic,  or  political 
problem  exists,  they  do  develop  attitudes  toward  that  problem: 
the  proportion  of  people  who  remain  neutral  or  who  have  no 
opinion  about  an  issue  they  are  aware  of  is  very  small  indeed. 


80     ATTITUDE  FORMULATION  EXTENDED  TO  CONCRETE  SITUATIONS 

Studies  on  the  prediction  of  social  events  have  indicated  the 
enormous  extent  to  which  attitudes  direct  the  way  in  which  the 
future  is  projected.  [21,  67,  83]  A  person's  attitude  toward  social- 
ism, for  example,  largely  determines  his  picture  of  future  economic 
trends.  But  the  more  highly  structured  and  clear-cut  the  forces 
determining  the  resolution  of  an  issue  appear  to  be,  the  less  is  an 
individual's  prediction  determined  by  his  own  attitude.  Predic- 
tions are  especially  influenced  by  frames  or  points  of  reference 
when  the  issue  judged  is  ambiguous  or  unstructured  because  of 
the  variety  and  apparent  inconsistency  of  direction  of  the  variables 
concerned. 

Although  the  bulk  of  our  attitudes  do  seem  to  be  derived  from 
the  norms  that  surround  us,  the  fact  remains  that — all  other  things 
being  ^^/—individuals  do  acquire  attitudes  based  on  knowledge 
and  reasoned  analysis.  Murphy  and  Likert  found,  for  example, 
that,  next  to  parental  influence,  a  student's  reading  habits  and 
scholarship  tended  more  than  other  factors  to  affect  his  atti- 
tudes. [70]  It  has  been  shown  that  there  was  a  tremendous  differ- 
ence between  enlightened  and  unenlightened  Americans  in  their 
pre-Pearl  Harbor  attitudes  toward  U.  S.  intervention  in  the  war — 
with  enlightenment  being  based  on  general  knowledge  and  a 
feeling  of  a  clear  idea  of  what  the  war  is  all  about.  [27]  Differ- 
ences of  opinion  according  to  the  degree  of  "enlightenment"  were 
found  within  all  income  groups  and  were  considerably  greater 
than  differences  of  opinion  between  income  groups  themselves. 
Evidence  from  public  opinion  polls  shows  that  well-informed 
people  accept  less  readily  than  uninformed  persons  many  of  the 
common  stereotypes  of  the  day,  the  facts  they  know  serving  as 
reference  points  for  discrimination.  [91]  For  example,  Walsh 
[89]  analyzed  the  relationship  between  the  amount  of  information 
the  American  people  had  about  the  Soviet  Union  and  their  con- 
fidence in  Russian  postwar  co-operation.  Information  appeared 
as  a  much  more  decisive  determinant  of  opinion  than  did  eco- 
nomic status  or  religion.  He  found  that,  in  the  fall  of  1944,  73 
per  cent  of  those  who  were  best  informed  trusted  Russia  to  co- 
operate with  the  United  States,  whereas  only  30  per  cent  of  those 
least  informed  trusted  Russia.  More  highly  educated  people 
appear  better  able  to  see  the  implications  of  a  point  of  view  they 


ATTITUDE  FORMULATION  EXTENDED  TO  CONCRETE  SITUATIONS      81 

hold  and  are  more  consistent  in  their  attitudes  than  are  less  edu- 
cated persons.  A  study  of  American  attitudes  toward  freedom  of 
speech,  made  during  the  critical  summer  of  1940,  indicated  that, 
although  well-educated  people  were  somewhat  more  opposed  to 
free  speech  at  that  time  than  less  well-educated  persons,  among  all 
those  who  did  favor  free  speech  the  well-educated  were  more 
willing 

...  to  extend  their  tolerance  to  the  logical  conclusion  of  allowing 
Fascists  and  Communists  to  speak.  Such  a  finding  seems  to  indicate 
that  these  people  are  more  burdened  by  the  intellectual  demand  for 
consistency  than  are  persons  who  have  had  less  educational  oppor- 
tunity. [27, 183  f.] 

Informed  people  are  better  able  than  uninformed  to  see  the 
implications  to  their  own  self-interest  of  events  and  proposals. 
They  show  considerably  more  concern,  for  example,  with  interna- 
tional affairs  as  contrasted  to  relatively  uninformed  persons  whose 
predominant  concern  is  with  strictly  domestic  problems.  [28] 
The  uninformed  group  generally  has  a  comparatively  higher  "no 
opinion"  reaction  to  most  public  questions.  Differences  of  opinion 
according  to  the  amount  of  information  a  person  has  do  not 
appear,  however,  with  respect  to  issues  where  wish  fulfillment  is 
clearly  involved.  For  example,  a  study  of  the  relative  importance 
of  education  and  economic  status  in  determining  opinion  showed 
that  education  was  more  important  than  income  only  in  situations 
where  greater  knowledge  gives  an  insight  into  the  effect  of  certain 
events  or  proposals  that  do  not  deal  with  an  individual's  financial 
return  in  any  clear-cut  way.  [92]  Diven's  analysis  of  aesthetic 
appreciation  indicates  that— all  other  things  being  equal— if  a  well- 
informed  and  an  uninformed  person  hold  the  same  attitude  with 
equal  intensity  the  essential  difference  between  them  is  that  the 
informed  man  will  be  better  able  to  rationalize  his  point  of  view, 
supporting  it  with  what  seems  to  him  sound  evidence.  [33] 

A  study  of  the  nationwide  panic  in  the  United  States  resulting 
from  Orson  Welles's  broadcast,  "War  of  the  Worlds,"  showed  that 
when  individuals  are  faced  with  a  critical  and  apparently  danger- 
ous situation  they  readily  accept  any  interpretation  offered  them 
as  a  basis  for  judgment  if  they  have  no  appropriate  and  sure 


82      ATTITUDE  FORMULATION  EXTENDED  TO  CONCRETE  SITUATIONS 

standards  by  means  of  which  to  evaluate  the  situation.  [23]  Those 
frightened  by  the  broadcast  were  highly  suggestible,  believing 
what  they  heard  to  be  true  and  being  unable  themselves  to  make 
any  external  or  internal  checks  of  the  reports.  Furthermore,  for 
most  of  those  who  became  panicked,  the  story  was  credible  since 
it  fitted  into  pre-existing  attitudes  such  as  the  belief  that  God 
would  someday  destroy  our  planet,  or  an  attack  by  a  foreign 
power,  or  fanciful  notions  concerning  the  possibilities  of  science. 
People  who  lacked  appropriate  standards  to  interpret  the  broad- 
cast properly  were  found  particularly  among  those  who  had  neither 
the  opportunity  nor  the  ability  to  acquire  information  or  training 
that  would  have  protected  them  with  relevant  points  of  reference. 
This  broadcast  occurred  in  the  fall  of  1938  when  vast  numbers  of 
the  population  were  unusually  bewildered  by  the  prolonged  eco- 
nomic insecurity  they  had  experienced  and  by  the  precarious  and 
delicate  state  of  world  affairs  following  the  Munich  settlement. 
In  commenting  on  the  panic,  Heywood  Broun  tersely  and  aptly 
summarized  this  effect  of  general,  social,  and  political  unrest  by 
saying  that  "jitters  have  come  to  roost."  Current  norms  were 
somehow  proving  inadequate  to  account  for  objective  conditions; 
the  whole  course  of  recent  history  created  a  relatively  ambiguous 
and  unstructured  situation  conducive  to  high  suggestibility. 

And  just  as  critical  conditions  provide  fertile  soil  for  panics,  so 
too  do  they  provide  the  optimum  conditions  for  rumor  or  for 
individual  reorientation  by  means  of  slogans  or  simple  appeals, 
Knapp  concludes,  for  example,  from  his  study  of  rumors  that  "in 
proportion  as  the  cognitive  world  is  ambiguous  or  ill  defined  and 
the  motivation  intense,  rumors  will  find  life,"  [57,  31]  F.  H. 
Allport  and  Lepkin  in  their  study  of  rumors  point  out  that 

The  more  "outer"  facts,  or  true  reports  of  facts,  the  individual  has 
within  his  grasp,  and  the  more  he  is  stimulated  to  weigh  this  evidence 
objectively,  the  more  nearly  the  picture  he  forms  "in  his  mind"  will 
conform  to  the  true  reality,  and  the  less  altered  it  will  be  through  the 
effect  of  emotion  and  impulse.  [3, 14] 

And  Sherif  points  out,  from  his  analysis  of  slogans, 

.  .  .  that  slogans  are  short-cut  expressions  arising  in  confused  and 
critical  situations  ...  the  more  correctly  and  the  more  objectively  a 


FRAME  OF  REFERENCE  IN  SOME  RECENT  ECONOMIC  WORKS  83 

set  of  slogans  expresses  the  underlying  forces  in  a  critical  situation,  the 
more  vital  and  lasting  they  will  prove  to  be.  [81,  461] 

In  a  more  recent  study  of  slogans,  Bellak  reaches  the  same  conclu- 
sion with  special  emphasis  on  the  role  of  motivational  factors.  [10] 
Since  slogans  can  only  be  expected  to  take  hold  in  critical  times 
when  they  give  meaning  to  or  point  a  way  out  of  confused  situa- 
tions, it  is  not  surprising  that  the  judgments  of  slogans  in  die 
laboratory  are  unaffected  by  conflicting  standards,  as  in  the  experi- 
ment of  Asch,  Block,  and  Hertzman  [6],  or  Block's  later  study 
where  only  restricted  shifts  were  found  in  the  ratings  of  slogans 
when  authoritative  standards  were  imposed  under  laboratory  con- 
ditions. [64]  The  leaders  of  any  mass  movement  or  revolution 
show  sound  psychology  when,  during  a  critical  situation  where 
old  norms  have  lost  their  hold,  they  try  first  of  all  to  get  control  of 
the  mass  media  of  communication  so  they  can  issue  new  instruc- 
tions, spread  slogans,  and  otherwise  try  to  restructure  people's 
thinking. 

Analysis  of  the  rise  of  various  social  movements  shows  how 
persons  dissatisfied  with  their  status  or  the  fulfillment  of  their 
needs  tend  to  accept  new  frames  of  reference  provided  by  a  leader 
or  nuclear  group  which  seem  to  them  to  "explain"  their  situation 
more  appropriately  and  to  offer  an  apparently  more  effective  course 
of  action  than  did  adherence  to  the  more  commonly  accepted 
norms  of  the  social  system.  [24]  The  suggestibility  of  a  person  to 
new  norms  was  found  to  be  proportional  to  the  inadequacy  of 
existing  standards  for  the  interpretation  of  particular  situations, 
to  his  desire  for  a  more  adequate  interpretation,  or  to  the  ease  with 
which  the  norms  of  a  new  movement  could  be  related  to  estab- 
lished frames. 

FRAME  OF  REFERENCE  IN  SOME  RECENT  ECONOMIC  WORKS 

Since  the  1930's,  economists  in  capitalistic  countries  have  been 
introducing  psychological  concepts  into  their  construction  of  a 
"dynamic"  economic  theory,  as  contrasted  to  the  more  "timeless" 
classical  economics.  They  are  bringing  out  into  the  open  for  close 
scrutiny  what  had  only  been  implicit  assumptions  of  more  static 


84      ATOTUDE  FORMULATION  EXTENDED  TO  CONCRETE  SITUATIONS 

theories:  assumptions  centering  around  the  concept  of  "expecta- 
tions." And  the  expectations  discussed  by  modern  economists  are 
frames  of  reference  that  determine  or  may  in  some  way  affect 
response  to  economic  conditions. 

The  English  economist,  Shackle,  in  his  Expectations,  Invest- 
ment,  and  Income  [78]  writes  that 

The  task  of  dynamic  economics  is  to  describe  the  inherent  character 
of  an  economy  in  such  a  way  that,  given  the  particular  situation  exist- 
ing at  one  moment,  as  to  the  conceptions  of  the  future  held  by  differ- 
ent individuals  and  the  composition  of  the  material  equipment,  it  is 
possible  to  deduce  the  situation  which  will  ensue,  if  there  are  no 
abnormal  extra-economic  impacts,  after  some  arbitrary  interval.  At 
any  one  moment  the  expectations  of  a  business  man,  which  deter- 
mine the  decision  he  makes  at  this  moment  as  to  his  action  in  the 
immediate  future,  are  given.  The  totality  of  action  by  all  business 
men  in  a  short  interval  depends,  if  we  take  this  interval  short  enough, 
on  new  decisions  taken  or  old  decisions  left  in  force  at  the  beginning 
of  this  interval.  The  totality  of  action  in  such  an  interval  thus  depends 
on  the  sets  of  expectations  held  by  different  business  men  at  the 
beginning  of  the  interval,  and  these  are  given.  [78, 1] 

Keynes  made  the  concept  of  expectations  central  in  his  The 
General  Theory  of  Employment,  Interest,  and  Money  which  had 
such  a  profound  influence  on  contemporary  economic  thinking. 

All  production  is  for  the  purpose  of  ultimately  satisfying  a  consumer. 
Time  usually  elapses,  however — and  sometimes  much  time — between 
the  incurring  of  costs  by  the  producer  (with  the  consumer  in  view) 
and  the  purchase  of  the  output  by  the  ultimate  consumer.  Meanwhile 
the  entrepreneur  (including  both  the  producer  and  the  investor  in  this 
description)  has  to  form  the  best  expectations  he  can  as  to  what  the 
consumers  will  be  prepared  to  pay  when  he  is  ready  to  supply  them 
(directly  or  indirectly)  after  the  lapse  of  what  may  be  a  lengthy 
period;  and  he  has  no  choice  but  to  be  guided  by  these  expectations, 
if  he  is  to  oroduce  at  all  by  processes  which  occupy  time.  [56, 46,  italics 
ours] 

After  distinguishing  between  two  groups  of  expectations,  short- 
term  and  long-term,  Keynes  points  out  that  "express  reference  to 
long-term  expectations  can  seldom  be  avoided."  [50]  These  long- 


FRAME  OF  REFERENCE  IN  SOME  RECENT  ECONOMIC  WORKS          85 

term  expectations  are  based  on  whatever  data  are  available  as  well 
as  on  the  confidence  with  which  any  forecast  is  made.4 

The  referential  nature  of  these  expectations  is  clearly  recognized. 
Shackle  [78],  for  example,  writes  that 

Before  we  can  use  Mr.  Keynes*  system  to  explain  the  economic 
pattern  which  emerges  with  the  passage  of  time,  we  must  release 
expectations  from  their  status  as  a  datum,  and  make  them  depend,  at 
any  moment,  on  the  comparison  which  we  may  suppose  business  men 
to  make  between  their  expectations  of  a  slightly  earlier  moment  and 
what  has  actually  happened  in  the  interval.  [78,  2] 

And  Shackle  points  out  in  a  later  article  that  it  is  necessary  to 
"study  the  psychology  of  expectations  as  a  process  of  the  individual 
mind."  [79,  99] 

This  concept  of  expectation  is  not  confined  merely  to  economic 
theory.  It  finds  its  place  in  concrete  affairs  of  everyday  life  as  is 
illustrated  in  the  report  of  a  Presidential  fact-finding  board  set  up 
to  review  the  General  Motors'  strike  in  the  United  States  in 
1946.  [73]  The  report  contains  such  phrases  as  these: 

On  balance,  we  thin\  it  reasonable  to  ex  feet  that  in  the  early  months 
of  the  new  period  productivity  will  be  somewhat  less  than  in  1941. 
.  .  .  We  thin\  it  reasonable  to  suppose  that  in  the  year  1946,  once  this 
strike  is  settled  and  production  begins  again  .  .  .  [73] 

And  the  President  of  General  Motors,  commenting  on  the  report 
of  the  fact-finding  board,  said  that  the  wage  recommendation 
made  "is  based  on  certain  assumptions  which  in  the  opinion  of 
General  Motors  are  unsound."  [74] 

The  Federal  Government  of  the  United  States  in  trying  to  for- 
mulate some  of  its  policies  is  relying  more  and  more  on  controlled 
investigations  of  expectations  so  policy  makers  will  have  some 
idea  of  what  the  future  behavior  of  certain  segments  of  the  popu- 

4Keynes's  clear  awareness  of  the  fact  that  the  economic  behavior  of  men  in 
bourgeois  society  occurs  within  a  limited  frame  of  reference  they  may  be  quite 
unaware  of  is  reflected  in  a  closing  passage  of  his  "General  Theory*':  "Practical 
men,  who  believe  themselves  to  be  quite  exempt  from  any  intellectual  influences, 
are  usually  the  slaves  of  some  defunct  economist  Madmen  in  authority,  who  hear 
voices  in  the  air,  are  distilling  their  frenzy  from  some  academic  scribbler  of  a  few 
years  back.  I  am  sure  that  the  power  of  vested  interests  is  vasdy  exaggerated 
compared  with  the  gradual  encroachment  of  ideas."  [56,  383] 


86      ATTITUDE  FORMULATION  EXTENDED  TO  CONCRETE  SITUATIONS 

lation  may  be.  For  example,  in  1942  and  1943,  public  expectations 
concerning  price  control  were  studied  for  the  Office  of  Price 
Administration  [48];  the  Board  of  Governors  of  the  Federal 
Reserve  System  used  public  opinion  surveys  because,  as  they  say, 

It  is  particularly  important  to  know  something  about  the  attitudes 
taken  by  liquid  asset  holders  toward  their  liquid  assets.  [37] 

And  the  U.  S.  Department  of  Agriculture  makes  periodic  checks 
on  farmers'  expectations,  because 

It  is  evident  that  the  planning  of  postwar  farm  programs  must 
depend  greatly  on  what  farmers  themselves  want  and  feel  they  must 
have.  [2,  ij 

Thus  government  officials  concerned  with  predictions  explicitly 
recognize  the  fact  that  behavior  can  be  predicted  only  if  the  frame 
of  reference  within  which  it  occurs  is  understood. 

This  relationship  between  the  concept  of  expectations  as  used  by 
economists  and  the  psychological  concept  of  frame  of  reference 
has  been  made  explicit  by  George  Katona.  [46,  47,  49]  He  has 
pointed  out  that  "the  frame  of  reference  of  important  economic 
groups"  plays  a  large  part  "in  determining  economic  decisions  and 
actions."  [47,  340]  He  notes  the  importance  of  determining 
whether  expectations  are  based  on  rumors  or  on  facts  and  empha- 
sizes that  the  understanding  and  prediction  of  economic  behavior 
can  be  precise  only  if  the  frame  of  reference  used  is  sufficiently 
encompassing  to  take  into  account  new  problems  and  situations 
brought  about  by  events. 

In  1938,  in  a  study  of  a  specific  social  movement  prominent  at 
the  time  (the  kingdom  of  Father  Divine),  we  indicated  how  this 
cult  could  be  regarded  as  a  microcosm  within  the  larger  world 
macrocosm,  how  conflicts  between  the  values  of  the  two  were 
inevitable  and  how  either  microcosms  must  "be  patterned  to  fit 
the  needs  of  an  individual  living  in  our  modern  world,  or  the 
conditions  in  the  larger  macrocosm  must  be  changed  to  provide 
the  satisfactions  and  meanings  now  artificially  derived  in  the 
microcosms."  [20, 166]  It  is  significant  to  see  the  same  distinction 
made  by  modern  economists  and  to  read  that  if  economic  analysis 


FRAME  OF  REFERENCE  IN  SOME  RECENT  ECONOMIC  WORKS  87 

is  to  be  "truly  dynamic,"  the  relationship  of  a  "section  of  the  huge 
economic  mechanism"  must  be  related  to  "fluctuations  of  the  whole 
economic  system  taken  in  its  entirety."  Frisch  wrote  in  1933: 

When  we  approach  the  study  of  a  business  cycle  with  the  intention 
of  carrying  through  an  analysis  that  is  truly  dynamic  ...  we  are 
naturally  led  to  distinguish  between  two  types  of  analyses:  the  micro- 
dynamic  and  the  macro-dynamic  types.  The  micro-dynamic  analysis 
is  an  analysis  by  which  we  try  to  explain  in  some  detail  the  behaviour 
of  a  certain  section  of  the  huge  economic  mechanism,  taking  for 
granted  that  certain  general  parameters  are  given.  Obviously  it  may 
well  be  that  we  obtain  more  or  less  cyclical  fluctuations  in  such  sub- 
systems, even  though  the  general  parameters  are  given.  The  essence 
of  this  type  of  analysis  is  to  show  the  details  of  the  evolution  of  a 
given  specific  market,  the  behaviour  of  a  given  type  of  consumers, 
and  so  on. 

The  macro-dynamic  analysis,  on  the  other  hand,  tries  to  give  an 
account  of  the  fluctuations  of  the  whole  economic  system  taken  in  its 
entirety.  Obviously  in  this  case  it  is  impossible  to  carry  through  the 
analysis  in  great  detail.  [41,  772] 

All  of  the  representative  studies  cited  indicate  that  only  by  means 
of  some  frame  or  anchorage  can  and  does  the  individual  judge  and 
react  to  social  stimuli.  Individuals  in  social  life  cannot  long  re- 
main normally  adjusted  if  they  are  in  a  state  of  indecision.  Sooner 
or  later  they  must  and  do  make  some  appropriate  judgment  or 
reaction  to  a  stimulus,  place  it  in  some  way  meaningful  to  them. 
The  psychological  process  they  use  to  make  such  judgments  and 
give  meaning  to  their  social  environment  is  to  refer  the  stimuli 
around  them  to  some  frame  of  reference  or  anchorage  they  acquire 
and  which  is  a  readiness  for  reaction.  These  frames  or  anchorages 
which  regulate  everyday  social  judgments  to  an  important  degree, 
like  frames  or  anchorages  discovered  in  laboratory  experiments, 
inevitably  develop  with  repeated  exposure  to  the  same  stimuli 
(objects,  persons,  groups,  values,  or  norms)  and  individuals  are  by 
no  means  always  aware  that  frames  or  anchorages  have  become 
established.  Furthermore,  as  we  indicate  in  later  chapters,  social 
attitudes  are  among  the  major  components  of  the  ego.  Hence  the 
psychology  of  ego-involvement  and  its  relation  to  motivation  can 


88      ATTITUDE  FORMULATION  EXTENDED  TO  CONCRETE  SITUATIONS 

only  be  clearly  understood  with  reference  to  the  basic  properties 
of  frame  formation  and  the  implications  this  has  for  :udgment  and 
action. 

REFERENCES 

1.  ABROMSON,  F.,  Perception:  Further  Evidence  for  a  Tri factorial  Theory,  Honor's 

Thesis,  Harvard  Univ.,  Dcpt.  Psychol.,  1946. 

2.  AGRICULTURE,  U.  S.  DEPARTMENT  OF,  Farmers  View  the  Postwar  World,  Bur. 

Agricultural  Economics,  September  25,  1944. 

3.  ALLPORT,  F.  H.,  and  M.  LEPKIN,  Wartime  rumors  of  waste  and  special  privi- 

lege, /.  Abnorm.  and  Soc.  Psychol..  1945,  40,  3-36. 

4.  ALLPORT,  G.  W.,  and  B.  M.  KRAMER,  Some  roots  of  prejudices,  /.  Psychol. , 

1946,  22,  9-39. 

5.  ANSBACHER,  H.,  Perception  of  number  as  affected  by  the  monetary  value  of  the 

objects:  a  critical  study  of  the  method  used  in  the  extended  constancy  phe- 
nomenon, Arch.  Psychol.,  1937,  30,  no.  215. 

6.  ASCH,  S.  E.,  H.  BLOCK,  and  M.  HERTZMAN,  Studies  in  the  principles  of  judg- 

ments and  attitudes:  I.  Two  basic  principles  of  judgment,  /.  Psychol.,  1938, 
5,  219-51. 

7.  ASCH,  S.  E.,  Studies  in  the  principles  of  judgments  and  attitudes:  II.  Determi- 

nation of  judgments  by  group  and  by  ego  standards,  /.  Soc.  Psychol.,  1940, 
12,  433-65. 

8.  BARTLETT,  F.  C.,  Remembering:  a  study  in  experimental  and  social  psychology, 

Cambridge  (England):  Univ.  Press,  copyright  1932. 

9.  BEAN,  L.  H.,  F.  MOSTELLER,  and  F.  WILLIAMS,  Nationalities  and  1944,  PubL 

Opin.  Quart.,  1944,  8,  368-75. 

10.  BELLAK,  L.,  The  nature  of  slogans,  /.  Abnorm.  &  Soc.  Psychol.,  1942,  37, 

496-510. 

11.  BIRCH,  H.  G.,  The  effect  of  socially  disapproved  labeling  upon  a  well-structured 

attitude,  /.  Abnorm.  &  Soc.  Psychol.,  1945,  40,  301-10. 

12.  BLAKE,  R.,  and  W.  DENNIS,  The  development  of  stereotypes  concerning  the 

Negro,  /.  Abnorm.  &  Soc.  Psychol.,  1943,  38,  525-31. 

13.  BORING,  E.  G.,  Sensation  and  Perception  in  the  History  of  Experimental  Psy- 

chology, New  York:  Appleton-Century,  1942. 

14.  BRESLAW,  B.  J.,  The  development  of  a  socio-economic  attitude,  Arch.  Psychol., 

1938,  no.  226. 

15.  BRUNER,  J.  S.,  Mandate  from  the  People,  New  York:  Duell,  Sloan  &  Pearce, 

1944. 

16. Social  value  and  need  as  organizing  factors  in  perception,  Amer.  Psy- 
chologist, 1946, 1,  241. 

17.  BRUNSWIK,  E.,  Wahrnehmung  und  Gegcnstandswclt,  Leipzig:  Deuticke,  1934. 

18.  CANTRIL,  H.,  General  and  specific  attitudes,  Psychol.  Monogr.,  1932,  no.  192. 
19. -•  Experimental  studies  of  prestige  suggestion,  Psychol.  Bull.,  1937,  34,  528. 

20.  : and  M.  SHERIF,  The  kingdom  of  Father  Divine,  /.  Abnorm.  &  Sac. 

PsychoL,  1938,  33, 147-67, 

21.  ! The  prediction  of  social  events,  /.  Abnorm.  &  Soc.  Psychol,  1938,  33, 

364-89. 


REFERENCES  89 

22.  CANTRIL,  H.,  The  role  of  the  radio  commentator,  Publ.  Opin.  Quart^  1939,  3, 
654-62. 

23. The  Invasion  from  Mars,  Princeton:  Univ.  Press,  1940. 

24. The  Psychology  of  Social  Movements,  New  York:  Wiley,  1941. 

25. and  J.  HARDING,  The  1942  elections:  a  case  study  in  political  psychology, 

Publ.  Opin.  Quart.,  1943,  7,  222-41. 

26. The  use  of  trends,  in  Gauging  Public  Opinion  (H.  Cantril,  ed.),  Prince- 
ton: Univ.  Press,  1944,  ch.  16. 

27. The  use  of  breakdowns,  in  Gauging  Public  Opinion  (H.  Cantril,  ed.), 

Princeton:  Univ.  Press,  1944,  ch.  13. 

28.  — • —  The  issues — as  seen  by  the  American  people,  Publ.  Opin.  Quart.,  1944, 

8,  331-47. 

29.  CENTERS,  R.,  and  H.  CANTRIL,  Income  satisfaction  and  income  aspiration, 

/.  Abnorm.  &  Soc.  Psychol.f  1946,  41,  64-9. 

30.  CHAPMAN,  D.  W.,  and  J.  VOLKMANN,  A  social  determinant  of  the  level  of 

aspiration,  /.  Abnorm.  6-  Soc.  Psychol.,  1939,  34,  225-38. 

31.  COFFIN,  T.  E.,  Some  conditions  of  suggestion  and  suggestibility:  a  study  of 

some  attitudinal  and  situational  factors  influencing  the  process  of  suggestion, 
Psychol.  Monogr.,  1941,  no.  241. 

32.  DAVIS,  J.,  Testing  the  social  attitudes  of  children  in  the  government  schools  in 

Russia,  Am.  J.  Sociolf  1927,  32,  947-52. 

33.  DIVEN,  K.,  Aesthetic  appreciation  test,  in  Explorations  in  Personality  (H.  A. 

Murray  et  al.),  New  York:  Oxford,  1938,  447-53. 

34.  EDWARDS,  A.  L.,  Political  frames  of  reference  as  a  factor  influencing  recogni- 

tion, /.  Abnorm.  &  Soc.  Psychol.,  1941,  36,  34-50. 
35. Rationalization  in  recognition  as  a  result  of  a  political  frame  of  reference, 

/.  Abnorm.  &  Soc.  Psychol.,  1941,  36,  224-35. 
36. Unlabeled  fascist  attitudes,  /.  Abnorm.  &  Soc.  Psychol.,  1941,  36,  575-82, 

37.  FEDERAL  RESERVE  SYSTEM,  Surveys  of  Liquid  Asset  Holdings,  Board  of  Gov- 

ernors, September  1945. 

38.  FERNBERGER,  S.  W.,  Instructions  and  the  psychophysical  limen,  Am.  J.  Psychol., 

1931,43,361-76. 

39.  FRANKLIN,  J.  H.,  The  Free  Negro  in  North  Carolina,  1790-1860,  Chapel  Hill: 

Univ.  N.  Carolina  Press,  copyright  1943. 

40.  FRIED,  E.,  and  H.  CANTRIL,  The  meaning  of  questions,  in  Gauging  Publ.  Opin. 

(H.  Cantril,  ed.),  Princeton:  Univ.  Press,  1944,  ch.  1. 

41.  FRISCH,  R.,  Propagation  problems  and  impulse  problems  in  dynamic  economics, 

in  Economic  Essays  in  Honour  of  Gustav  Cassel,  London:  Allen  and  Unwin, 
copyright  1933. 

42.  GOODMAN,  C.  C.,  Perception:  Evidence  for  a  Trifactorial  Theory,  Honor's 

Thesis,  Harvard  Univ.,  Dept.  Psychol.,  1946. 

43.  HARTLEY,  R.,  and  E.  HARTLEY,  Analysis  of  the  judgment  of  pictures  of  people 

in  a  poorly  defined  situation,  to  be  published. 

44.  HARTMANN,  G.  W.,  The  contradiction  between  the  feeling-tone  of  political 

party  names  and  public  response  to  their  platforms,  /.  Soc.  Psychol. f  1936,  7, 
336-57, 

45.  HOROWITZ,  E.  L.,  The  development  of  attitude  toward  the  Negro,  'Arch. 

Psychol.,  1936,  no.  194. 

46.  KATONA,  G.,  War  Without  Inflation,  New  York:  Columbia  Univ.,  1942. 


90      ATTITUDE  FORMULATION  EXTENDED  TO  CONCRETE  SITUATIONS 

47.  KATONA,  G.,  The  role  of  the  frame  of  reference  in  war  and  post-war  economy, 
Am.  I  Social,  1944,  49,  340-7. 

48. Price  Control  and  Business,  Bloomington,  Ind.:  Principia  Press,  1945. 

49. Psychological  analysis  of  business  decisions  and  expectations,  Am.  Econ. 

Rev.,  1946,  36,  44-62. 

50.  KATZ,  D.,  and  F.  H.  ALLPORT,  Students'  Attitudes,  Syracuse:  Craftsman,  1931. 

51. and  K.  BRALY,  Racial  stereotypes  of  one  hundred  college  students,  /.  Ab- 

norm.  &  Soc.  Psychol.,  1933,  28,  280-90. 

52. Racial  prejudice  and  racial  stereotypes,  /.  Abnorm.  &  Soc.  Psychol., 

1935,  30, 175-93. 

53. and  H.  CANTRIL,  An  analysis  of  attitudes  toward  fascism  and  com- 
munism, /.  Abnorm.  &  Soc.  Psychol.,  1940,  35,  356-66. 

54.  KAY,  L.  W.,  The  relation  of  personal  frames  of  reference  to  social  judgments, 

Arch.  Psychol.,  1943,  no.  283. 

55.  Social  norms  as  determinants  in  the  interpretation  of  personal  experi- 
ences, /.  Soc.  Psychol.,  1944,  19,  359-67. 

56.  KEYNES,  J.  M.,  The  General  Theory  of  Employment,  Interest,  and  Money,  New 

York:  Harcourt,  Brace,  copyright  1936. 

57.  KNAPP,  R.,  A  psychology  of  rumor,  Publ.  Opin.  Quart.,  1944,  8,  22-37. 

58.  KORNHAUSER,  A.  W.,  Attitudes  of  economic  groups,  Publ.  Opin.  Quart.,  1938, 

2,  260-8. 

59. Analysis  of  "class"  structure  of  contemporary  American  society — psycho- 
logical bases  of  class  divisions,  in  Industrial  Conflict:  A  Psychological  Inter- 
pretation (G.  W.  Hartmann  and  T.  Newcomb,  eds.),  New  York:  Cordon, 
1939, 199-264. 

60.  LASKER,  B.,  Race  Attitudes  in  Children,  New  York:  Holt,  1929. 

61.  LAZARSFELD,  P.  F.,  Report  on  "Naples  is  a  battleground,"  unpublished,  June 

1944. 

62.  B.  BERELSON,  and  H.  GAUDET,  The  People's  Choice:  How  the   Voter 

Ma\es  Up  His  Mind  in  a  Presidential  Campaign,  New  York:  Duell,  Sloan  & 
Pearce,  1944. 

63.  LEVINE,  J.  M.,  and  G.  MURPHY,  The  learning  and  forgetting  of  controversial 

material,  /.  Abnorm.  &  Soc.  Psychol.,  1943,  38,  507-17. 

64.  LEWIS,  H.  B.,  Studies  in  the  principles  of  judgments  and  attitudes:  II.  The 

influence  of  political  attitude  on  the  organization  and  stability  of  judgments, 
/.  Soc.  Psychol.,  1940,  11,  121-46. 

65.  LIPPMANN,  W.,  Public  Opinion,  New  York:  Harcourt,  Brace,  1922. 

66.  MCGARVEY,  H.  R.,  Anchoring  effects  in  the  absolute  judgment  of  verbal  ma- 

terials, Arch.  Psychol.,  1943,  no.  281. 

67.  MCGREGOR,  D.,  The  major  determinants  of  the  prediction  of  social  events,  /. 

Abnorm.  &  Soc.  Psychol.,  1938,  33,  179-204. 

68.  MEENES,  M.,  A  comparison  of  racial  stereotypes  of  1935  and  1942,  /.  Soc. 

Psychol.,  1943, 17,  327-36. 

69.  MENEFEE,  S.  C.,  The  effect  of  stereotyped  words  on  political  judgments,  Am. 

Sociol.  Rev.,  1936, 1,  614-21. 

70.  MURPHY,  G.,  and  R.  LIKERT,  Public  Opinion  and  the  Individual,  New  York: 

Harper,  1938. 

71.  NEWCOMB,  T.  M.,  Personality  and  Social  Change,  New  York:  Dryden,  1943. 

72.  NATIONAL  OPINION  RESEARCH  CENTER,  Survey,  May  1944. 


REFERENCES  91 

73.  New  Yor{  Times,  January  11,  1946. 

74.  New  York.  Times,  January  13, 1946. 

75.  OSGOOD,  C.  E.,  and  R.  STAGNER,  Analysis  of  a  prestige  frame  of  reference  by  a 

gradient  technique,  /.  Appl.  PsychoL,  1941,  25,  275-90. 

76.  ROGERS,  S.,  The  anchoring  of  absolute  judgments,  Arch.  PsychoL,  1941,  no.  261. 

77.  SEELEMAN,  V.,  The  influence  of  attitude  upon  the  remembering  of  pictorial 

material,  Arch.  PsychoL,  1940,  no.  258. 

78.  SHACKLE,  G.  L.,  Expectations,  Investment,  and  Income,  London:  Oxford  Univ. 

Press,  copyright  1938. 

79. The  expectational  dynamics  of  the  individual,  Economica,  1943,  10,  99. 

80.  SHERIF,  M.,  A  study  of  some  social  factors  in  perception,  Arch.  PsychoL,  1935, 

no.  187. 

81. The  psychology  of  slogans,  /,  Abnorm.  &  Soc.  PsychoL,  1937,  32,  450-61. 

82.  SMITH,  G.  H.,  The  effects  of  fact  and  rumor  labels  on  the  belief  in  unverified 

"news"  statements,  to  be  published  in  /.  Abnorm.  &  Soc.  PsychoL,  1947. 

83. An  analysis  of  panel  predictions,  to  be  published. 

84.  STACKER,  R.,  Fascist  attitudes:  an  exploratory  study,  /.  Soc.  PsychoL,  1936,  7, 

309-19. 
85. A  correlational  analysis  of  nationalistic  opinions,  /.  Soc.  PsychoL,  1940, 

12, 197-212. 

86.  STERN,  W.,  Zur  Psychologic  der  Aussage,  Berlin,  1902. 
87. The  psychology  of  testimony,  /.  Abnorm.  &  Soc.  PsychoL,  1939,  34,  3-20. 

88.  VETTER,  G.  B.,  Measurement  of  social  and  political  attitudes  and  the  related 

personality  factors,  /.  Abnorm.  &  Soc.  PsychoL,  1930,  25,  149-89. 

89.  WALSH,  W.  B.,  What  the  American  people  think  of  Russia,  Publ.  Opin.  Quart., 

1944,  8,  513-22. 

90.  WATSON,  W.  S.,  and  G.  W.  HARTMANN,  Rigidity  of  a  basic  attitudinal  frame, 

/.  Abnorm.  &•  Soc.  PsychoL,  1939,  34,  314-36. 

91.  WILLIAMS,  F.  W.,  Information  as  a  determinant  of  opinion,  in  Gauging  Public 

Opinion  (H.  Cantril,  ed.),  Princeton:  Univ.  Press,  1944,  ch.  15. 

92. and  F.  MOSTELLER,  Education  and  economic  status  as  determinants  of 

opinion,  in  Gauging  Public  Opinion  (H.  Cantril,  ed.),  Princeton:  Univ. 
Press,  1944,  ch.  14. 

93.  WOOD,  C.,  An  analysis  of  changes  occurring  in  successive  stages  of  verbal  ab- 

stracting, Master's  thesis  on  file  in  the  library  of  the  State  Univ.,  Iowa,  1944. 

94.  WOODWORTH,  R.  S.,  Situation-and-goal  set,  in  Am.  /.  PsychoL,  Golden  Jubilee 

vol.  (K.  M.  Dallenbach,  ed.),  1937,  50,  13(MO. 

95.  ZILLIG,  M.,  Einstellung  und  Aussage,  Z.  PsychoL,  1928,  106,  58-106. 


CHAPTER  5 

THE  PROBLEM  AND  A  GENERAL  CHARACTERIZATION 
OF  EGO-INVOLVEMENTS 

In  this  chapter  we  briefly  sketch  the  problem  and  give  a  general 
characterization  of  ego-involvements.  Our  characterization  here 
necessarily  consists  of  rather  arbitrary  statements.  These  will  stand 
or  fall  in  the  light  of  evidence  presented  in  succeeding  chapters. 

As  we  said  in  the  introduction,  we  are  using  the  word  "ego" 
reluctantly.  For  the  term  has  acquired  many  scientifically  objec- 
tionable connotations,  especially  in  the  hands  of  certain  philoso- 
phers and  mystically  inclined  writers.  The  "pure  ego"  posited  by 
William  James,  for  example,  is  one  of  these  connotations  which 
has  no  psychological  meaning  for  us,  even  though  James  did  in 
other  respects  discuss  the  problem  of  the  ego  in  naturalistic  terms. 
Nor  is  the  word  "self"  devoid  of  these  objectionable  associations. 
And  inasmuch  as  some  religiously  inclined  psychologists  are  cur- 
rently grasping  the  term  "ego"  as  a  substitute  for  "soul,"  there 
might  be  some  advantage  for  scientific  psychology  if  the  concept 
were  designated  by  Greek  symbols.  But  we  have  preferred  not  to 
do  this  and  have  contented  ourselves  by  using  "ego"  in  a  strictly 
demonstrable  way  on  the  basis  of  the  observed  facts  of  experience 
and  behavior. 

As  will  become  evident  in  the  chapters  to  follow,  what  is  called 
the  "ego"  consists,  in  the  last  analysis,  of  a  constellation  of  attitudes 
which  can  be  designated  as  ego-attitudes.  Since  scientific  studies 
on  ego-involvements  are  still  in  their  initial  stages,  we  are  not  yet 
ready  in  this  book  on  ego-involvements  to  give  a  clear-cut  defini- 
tion of  the  ego.  But  as  we  go  along  we  shall  give  concrete  and 
specific  cases  of  constellations  of  ego-attitudes.  And  in  passing  we 
may  point  again  to  the  fact,  elaborated  later,  that  the  ego  is  not  a 
fixed  and  immutable  entity.  It  is  formed  during  the  genetic 
development  of  the  individual  (ch.  7);  it  is  subject  to  transforma- 

92 


THE  PROBLEM  OF  EGO-INVOLVEMENTS  93 

tion  (see  especially  ch.  9)  and  to  disintegration  under  the  stress  of 
diverse  factors  (ch.  12). 

We  can  perhaps  take  as  a  matter  of  everyday  experience  the  fact 
that  most  of  the  affective  fixations  (attitudes)  which  determine, 
delimit,  focus,  and  shape  the  selectivity  of  experience  and  response 
to  various  stimulus  situations  are  connected  with  the  individuals, 
situations,  or  institutions  to  which  a  person  is  in  some  way  related. 
Attitudes  are  toward  my  parents,  my  school,  my  gang,  my  church, 
my  nation,  my  boss,  my  friend;  toward  my  friend's  rival,  my 
father's  competitor,  my  country's  enemy,  and  so  on.  Most  atti- 
tudes have  the  characteristic  of  belonging  to  me,  as  being  part  of 
me,  as  psychologically  experienced.  In  short,  they  are,  to  repeat 
Koffka's  term,  "Ego-attitudes." x  [6,  562] 

The  personal  world  of  every  individual  thus  becomes  centered 
around  himself,  as  William  Stern  has  pointed  out.  [19]  Without 
in  any  way  subscribing  to  the  implications  of  Stern's  personalistic 
psychology,  we  should  call  attention  to  his  observations  that  in 
making  judgments  of  "space"  and  "time"  the  individual  inevitably 
uses  himself  as  a  central  point  of  reference.  This  holds  for  what 
we  regard  as  "inside"  and  "outside"  of  ourselves,  what  we  regard 

xln  his  Principles  of  Gestah  Psychology  [6]  published  in  1935,  Koffka  intro- 
duced the  ego  as  "the  hero  of  the  play."  [319]  Following  the  line  of  general 
Gestah  psychology,  Koffka  conceived  of  the  ego  "as  a  field  object.**  [319]  In  his 
phenomenological  analysis  of  the  sensory  experiences  that  segregate  the  ego  from 
the  rest  of  the  environment,  Koffka  made  out  his  case  for  "the  reality  of  the  ego." 
In  this  and  in  later  chapters  (6  and  12)  we  refer  to  some  of  Koffka's  keen  obser- 
vations and  to  some  of  his  fresh  interpretations  of  the  experimental  psychology  he 
knew  so  well.  However,  Koffka's  formulation  of  the  ego  seems  to  us  quite  inade- 
quate for  systematic  psychology  and  especially  for  social  psychology,  largely  because 
of  his  neglect  of  the  characteristics  of  attitudes,  especially  those  concerned  with 
social  behavior,  and  the  functional  relationship  of  attitudes  to  the  ego.  He  re- 
garded attitudes  as  "forces  which  have  their  origin  not  in  the  surrounding  field 
at  all,  but  in  the  ego  of  the  observer"  [149,  italics  ours] ;  an  "attitude  is  a  force 
which  starts  in  the  ego."  [206,  italics  ours]  This  tendency  to  reify  the  ego  and 
to  conceive  of  it  as  an  entity  which  somehow  generates  and  propels  attitudes  is 
incompatible  with  our  own  position  and  seems  to  us  to  make  for  unnecessary 
obscurity  and  confusion.  And  Koffka  did  not  stress  either  the  fact  or  the 
implications  of  the  fact  that  the  ego  is  a  genetic  development. 

In  chapter  14  we  indicate  why  we  cannot  accept  the  psychoanalytic  interpretation 
of  the  ego.  .We  also  indicate  there  why  Adler's  conception  of  the  ego  as  an  end 
result  of  the  universal  "feeling  of  inferiority**  arid  the  compensating  "longing  for 
superiority"  seems  to  us  invalid. 


94  THE  PROBLEM  OF  EGO-INVOLVEMENTS 

as  "above"  and  "below,"  as  "before"  and  "behind,"  as  "left"  or 
"right,"  as  "future"  or  "past."  [97-97]  What  Stern  calls  a  person's 
"introceptible  world"  is  for  him  "the  cosmos  of  individualized 
carriers  of  significance  and  value,  and  of  claims  that  become  re- 
lated to  the  person's  center."  [88] 

We  have  considered  the  psychology  of  attitudes  in  some  detail 
in  the  three  previous  chapters  because  attitudes  are  the  main  con- 
stituents of  the  ego.  Most  attitudes  are  not  discrete  and  isolated 
entities  in  the  psychological  make-up  of  the  individual.  Once 
formed,  they  provide  the  major  components  of  which  the  ego  is 
built.  And  so  before  considering  the  problem  of  the  ego  itself  and 
the  characteristics  of  ego  functioning,  we  have  tried  to  make  sure 
that  our  conception  of  these  ego  components  rested  on  a  solid  basis 
of  fact.  For  this  reason  we  examined  experimental  evidence  from 
the  psychological  laboratory  and  evidence  from  more  concrete 
situations.  The  basic  psychology  of  ego  formation  is  essentially 
the  psychology  of  the  formation  of  attitudes.  But  we  must  wait 
for  the  accumulation  of  more  factual  evidence  before  we  are  in  a 
position  to  delineate  and  define  adequately  the  constellation  of  ego- 
attitudes  in  the  psychological  make-up  of  the  individual.  For  the 
constellation  of  ego-attitudes  is  not  coextensive  with  the  whole 
field  of  psychology. 

The  characteristics  of  attitudes  have  already  been  indicated  in 
previous  chapters.  Here  we  can  give  only  a  rough  indication  of 
the  implications  of  these  characteristics  for  the  psychology  of  ego- 
involvement.  More  elaborate  treatments  of  these  implications  as 
based  on  factual  data  are  contained  in  later  chapters.  In  chapter  2 
we  pointed  out  that  attitudes  have  a  more  or  less  enduring  quality. 
The  first  stage  in  the  actual  formation  of  an  attitude,  being  a  per- 
ceptual stage  and  involving  both  internal  and  external  factors, 
implies  that  attitude  formation  is  a  cognitive  process,  that  attitudes 
are  learned.  This  fact  takes  on  special  significance  as  we  consider 
the  relationship  between  attitudes  and  the  ego.  For  it  means  that 
the  characteristic  feeling  of  continuity  and  permanence  the  indi- 
vidual has  about  himself  as  revealed  by  the  constellation  of  ego- 
attitudes  is  derived  in  large  part  from  the  enduring  quality  of  the 
many  social  and  personal  attitudes  which  become  ego  components. 

This  does  not  imply,  as  we  have  noted  before,  that  attitudes  are 


THE  PROBLEM  OF  EGO-INVOLVEMENTS  95 

fixed  and  unchangeable  and,  as  a  consequence,  that  the  ego  is  fixed 
and  unchangeable.  The  ego  is  definitely  not  a  solidified  structure, 
permanent  once  it  is  formed.  Henri  Wallon,  in  his  excellent  study 
Lcs  Qrigmes  du  caractere  chez  lf  enfant  [20],  points  out  with  abun- 
dant evidence  the  changing  nature  of  the  small  child's  ego  relation- 
ships as  he  becomes  involved  in  different  situations.  "Personality 
remains  as  it  were  connected  with  the  situation."  (La  personnalite 
reste  comme  adherente  a  la  situation.  [247])  He  cites,  for  ex- 
ample, the  case  of  a  three-year-old  girl  who  had  spent  several 
weeks  in  the  country  with  her  mother.  When  her  father  arrived 
from  Vienna  and  asked  if  she  recognized  him,  she  said  that  her 
other  father  was  in  Vienna.  When  the  father  told  her  he  was  her 
daddy  from  Vienna,  she  asked  if  he  came  on  the  train.  Wallon 
indicates  that  the  child  needed  to  recall  her  own  trip,  the  change 
it  had  brought  about  in  her  own  situation,  before  she  could  bring 
the  two  fathers  into  focus  and  "resolve  in  this  way  the  incompati- 
bility of  the  situations  in  which  they  appeared  to  her  in  the  present 
and  in  the  past"  (resoudre  par  ce  moyen  rincompatibilite  des 
situations  ou  ils  lui  apparaissaient  actuellement  et  dans  le  passe 
[247]). 

We  shall  also  see  that  ego-attitudes  manifested  in  different  situa- 
tions can  be  quite  contradictory.  As  Healy  and  Bronner  have 
indicated  on  the  basis  of  their  studies  of  delinquent  children,  what 
we  call  "conscience,"  though  universally  found,  "plays  various  and 
partial  roles  in  determining  or  motivating  behavior."  [3,  12] 

Conscience  may  cover  only  certain  areas  in  the  field  of  conduct.  In 
one  case  of  our  series  a  young  boy  evidently  had  a  strong  conscience 
about  being  mannerly  and  doing  his  school  work  well,  while  stealing 
seemed  really  to  mean  nothing  to  him  except  as  he  might  be  caught 
for  it.  And  we  have  noted  in  some  instances  that  lying  was  quite 
condoned  by  conscience  while  stealing  was  a  sin,  and  that  in  other 
cases  this  was  exactly  reversed.  [3, 12] 

Later  we  find  ample  evidence  that  the  constellation  of  ego-atti- 
tudes can  and  does  change  in  a  given  individual  under  the  impact 
of  compelling  situations  (chs.  9-11).  When  we  consider  the  prob- 
lem of  ego-breakdowns,  we  shall  see  that  an  individual's  charac- 
teristic ego  structure  may  become  completely  dissociated,  may 


96  THE  PROBLEM  OF  EGO-INVOLVEMENTS 

entirely  collapse  under  cases  of  extreme  stress  or  pathological  con- 
ditions (ch.  12).  Other  attitudes  may  be  only  momentary  affairs, 
but  may  still  involve  the  ego.  The  accumulating  experiments 
related  to  ego-involvements  show  that  even  under  artificial  lab- 
oratory conditions  judgment  and  reaction  are  temporarily  altered 
in  situations  to  which  the  ego  is  linked  experimentally  (ch.  6). 

Ego-attitudes  may  be  formed  (learned)  in  relation  to  objects, 
persons,  situations,  or  groups  that  somehow  satisfy  or  frustrate 
those  basic  tensions,  needs,  drives,  or  instincts  that  do  have  a  defi- 
nitely assignable  locus  in  the  organism.  As  we  pointed  out  in 
chapter  2,  the  need  for  food  or  the  sexual  drive  as  such  might  be 
satisfied  by  an  individual  in  any  culture  by  almost  any  kind  of  food 
or  any  person  of  the  opposite  sex.  But  in  everyday  life  this  is  by 
no  means  the  usual  state  of  affairs.  We  acquire  tastes  for  special 
foods,  and  we  are  selective  in  our  choice  of  a  mate.  Under  or- 
dinary circumstances,  the  satisfaction  of  our  instinctual  urges  takes 
place  within  an  acquired  frame  of  reference  which  is  built  up 
through  experience  and  carries  definite  affective  overtones  as  our 
needs  are  satisfied  or  thwarted.  It  is  these  ego-involved  attitudes 
that,  to  a  large  extent  under  usual  conditions,  determine  and  direct 
the  particular  way  in  which  the  individual  goes  about  releasing  his 
instinctual  tendencies  and  drives. 

And  since  the  particular  manner  in  which  an  individual  satisfies 
his  instinctual  urges  becomes  in  most  societies  an  indicator  of 
status,  behavior  which  has  its  roots  in  biological  and  localizable 
functions  of  the  organism  becomes  inextricably  interwoven  with 
complex  ego-attitudes  learned  from  society.  A  particular  food — 
its  variety,  quality,  fashionableness,  where  and  how  it  is  eaten— 
can  be  and  is  used  as  a  mark  of  a  person's  position  in  life.  We  see 
later  that  clothing,  in  addition  to  the  function  it  serves  in  protect- 
ing the  body,  also  assumes  in  most  cultures  a  status-distinguishing 
role  (ch.  11).  We  will  indicate  the  enormous  consequences  for 
behavior  when  the  ego-attitudes  that  normally  direct  and  deter- 
mine the  way  in  which  an  individual  will  satisfy  instinctual  urges 
break  down  in  conditions  of  extreme  stress  or  deprivation  (ch.  12). 

All  attitudes  that  define  a  person's  status  or  that  give  him  some 
relative  role  with  respect  to  other  individuals,  groups,  or  institu- 
tions are  ego-involved.  Whether  these  attitudes  stem  from  some 


THE  PROBLEM  OF  EGO-INVOLVEMENTS  97 

biological  drive  or  whether  they  are  derived  from  some  non- 
instinctual  source  (from  some  social  value  or  norm),  any  attitude 
is  ego  linked  which  functions  to  shape,  delimit,  or  point  to  our 
relative  position.  In  the  United  States,  for  example,  the  individual 
derives  and  experiences  a  general  status  from  such  reference  groups 
as  white  or  Negro;  native  or  foreign  born;  upper,  middle,  or 
working  class;  worker,  employer,  or  independent;  Catholic  or 
Protestant.  And  one  derives  and  experiences  a  more  specific  status 
from  his  relative  position  in  membership  groups,  that  is,  as  the 
father  of  a  family,  a  newcomer  in  a  gang,  an  active  member  of  his 
union,  a  key  skilled  worker  in  a  factory.  Directories  such  as 
Who's  Who,  compiled  for  the  purpose  of  making  it  possible  "to 
place"  a  person,  describe  individuals  entirely  in  terms  of  their 
reference  and  membership  characters.2  And  these  attitudes,  which 
define  our  relative  position  and  status,  determine  and  regulate 
experience  and  action  in  a  major  way  in  all  those  situations  we 
relate  to  them. 

In  addition  to  the  evidence  already  presented,  we  will  see  in 
nearly  all  of  die  succeeding  chapters  how  these  ego-involved  atti- 
tudes—in youth  and  maturity,  in  everyday  life  and  in  the  labora- 
tory— guide  our  thoughts  and  actions  as  we  try  to  maintain  or 
improve  our  status  or  as  we  may  try  to  shift  it  with  changing 
conditions.  Our  satisfaction  with  ourselves,  our  aspirations  and 
ambitions,  our  failures  and  disappointments,  our  fears  and  anxie- 
ties are  all  relative  affairs,  relative  in  the  sense  that  we  inevitably 
use  ourselves,  our  position,  our  accomplishments,  as  reference 
points  against  the  scale  of  values  of  our  reference  and  membership 
groups. 

Since  the  ego  is  a  genetic  formation,  the  particular  attitudes  that 
become  its  major  components  will  vary  according  to  the  situations 
to  which  an  individual  is  exposed,  the  particular  constellation  of 
social  values  or  norms  which  have  served  as  stimulus  situations. 
As  a  result,  then,  the  ego  will  have  different  components  for  indi- 

2  We  are  not  ignoring  the  fact  that  we  also  think  of  ourselves  in  terms  of 
certain  temperamental  characteristics  or  abilities  we  may  possess  or  wish  we 
possessed.  A  discussion  of  these  personality  characteristics  and  their  place  in  social 
psychology  is  much  too  complicated  to  be  treated  here  and  must  be  reserved  for 
a  later  volume. 


98  THE  PROBLEM  OF  EGO-INVOLVEMENTS 

viduals  living  in  different  social  systems  or  cultures.  Observations 
of  anthropologists  reveal  the  enormous  variety  of  established  values 
or  norms  that  become  major  ego  constituents  for  individuals 
brought  up  in  different  cultures.  As  a  result  of  these  different  ego 
identifications,  very  diverse  loyalties  and  allegiances  exist.  Further- 
more, individuals  or  groups  or  classes  of  individuals  within  the 
same  society  may  have  quite  conflicting  or  opposing  values.  The 
ego  components  of  some  individuals  may  conflict  because  of  basic 
conflicts  they  reflect  in  objective  social  conditions  (chs.  10,  11). 
For  example,  those  who  find  themselves  members  of  a  minority 
group  in  a  larger  macrocosm,  those  who  occupy  marginal  positions 
in  a  society,  are  likely  to  form  contrasting  and  conflicting  ego 
identifications.  Ego  components  for  other  individuals  may  be 
more  overlapping  and  integrated,  owing  to  the  more  harmonious 
consistent  relationship  of  the  norms  of  a  society. 

By  no  means  all  of  the  values  incorporated  are  social  values  in 
the  sense  that  they  are  widely  accepted  norms.  Relatively  or  en- 
tirely unique  personal  values  derived  from  our  own  distinctive 
experience  may  and  often  do  become  important  components  of 
the  ego.  In  stressing  the  fact  that  major  e&o-attitudes  for  most 
individuals  seem  to  be  acquired  by  the  direct  acquisition  of  social 
values  or  norms,  we  do  not  mean  to  ignore  or  in  any  way  to 
minimize  the  fact  that  for  some  people  or  groups  of  people  ego- 
attitudes  can  be  and  are  derived  from  the  analysis  of  objective 
situations,  from  facts,  and  from  other  direct  experiences  which 
they  have  with  the  objects,  persons,  institutions,  and  such,  which 
become  the  content  of  attitudes.  If  this  were  not  the  case,  it  would 
be  more  difficult  than  it  is  to  account  for  the  political  radical,  the 
creative  musician,  artist,  or  scientist.  These  two  facts — namely 
that  the  content  of  ego-attitudes  need  not  be  always  derived  from 
widely  held  values  or  norms  and  that  they  may  be  built  up  through 
the  direct  observation,  experience,  and  synthesis  of  specific  facts, 
specific  objective  relationships — help  us  further  to  account  for  the 
enormous  individual  differences  we  see  in  man's  ego-strivings,  for 
the  different  definitions  people  give  to  "self-interest," 

Group  allegiances  can  and  do  direct  a  person  into  behavior 
which  appears  to  be  deviant  from  that  of  society  at  large.  We 
shall  see  how  the  complete  consistency  of  that  behavior  can  only 


THE  PROBLEM  OF  EGO-INVOLVEMENTS  99 

be  understood  in  terms  of  the  individual's  group  identifications 
(ch.  10).  There  is  unequivocal  evidence  that  the  adolescent  strives 
to  anchor  himself  with  some  reference  group  that  will  give  him 
general  status  or  with  some  particular  membership  group  in  which 
the  ego  can  be  anchored  more  specifically  (ch.  9). 

We  will  also  show  in  later  chapters  what  variety,  change,  and 
shifting  about  there  may  be  in  the  ego  components  of  the  same 
individuals  under  different  situations,  particularly  those  situations 
which  have  the  elements  of  crisis  or  conflict.  As  Koffka  has 
pointed  out,  and  as  becomes  increasingly  clear,  the  ego  is  complex 
[6, 333],  not  a  unidimensional  structure.  Koffka  noted  that  under 
varying  conditions  the  ego  will  "shrink,"  "contract,"  "expand," 
and  so  on.  Illustrations  of  this  variability  are  found  in  concrete 
social  situations  (ch.  11).  This  constellation  of  ego  components, 
shifting  under  different  situations,  has  been  recognized  by  novelists 
and  playwrights  (ch.  13). 

A  characteristic  fact  that  holds  for  any  individual  in  any  culture 
is  that  experiences  related  to  ego-attitudes,  ego  experiences,  are  felt 
by  the  individual  with  a  peculiar  warmth  and  familiarity.  This 
was  pointed  out  by  Katzaroff  in  1911  [5] : 

One  may  therefore  assume  that  the  feeling  of  familiarity  (sentiment 
dc  familier),  deja  vu,  which  accompanies  a  repeated  sensation  results 
from  the  fact  that  this  sensation,  when  it  passed  through  our  con- 
sciousness for  the  first  time  became  associated  with  the  very  feeling  of 
our  "Ego"  (s'est  associee  au  sentiment  lui-meme  de  notre  "moi")  and 
was,  so  to  speak,  enveloped  by  it.8  [5,  78] 

In  a  passage  referring  to  Freud's  concept  of  narcissism,  Murphy, 
Murphy,  and  Newcomb  have  indicated  their  own  belief  that  an 
individual's  perception  of  continuous  stimuli  contributes  to  the 
emergence  of  a  new  value,  the  self: 

As  the  primitive,  vague,  ill-defined  awareness  of  personal  identity 
gives  place  to  a  more  well-defined  experience,  as  the  individual  learns 
where  his  own  existence  stops  and  the  rest  of  the  world  begins,  the 
self  in  the  accepted  narrow  sense  is  born  and  becomes  an  empirical 
object  toward  which  the  attitudes  of  the  organism  are  built  up,  just  as 

8  The  reference  to  this  observation  and  the  English  translation  are  given  by 
Koffka.  [6,  594] 


100  THE  PROBLEM  OF  EGO-INVOLVEMENTS 

attitudes  are  built  up  toward  anything  else  ...  the  thing  known  as 
the  self  is  a  selection  and  organization  of  its  experiences  involving  the 
visceral  tensions,  muscular  strains,  the  sound  of  one's  name,  one's 
mirror  image,  and  so  on;  and  the  thing  which  knows  this  pattern  is 
simply  the  organism  as  a  whole.  If  this  is  correct,  it  is  easy  to  see  that 
the  self,  being  a  primary  source  of  many  satisfactions,  must  inevitably 
become  a  value.  The  self  is  something  which  we  like  and  from  which 
we  expect  much.  [13,  209  f.] 

It  is  also  the  constant  ego  reference  of  so  much  of  our  experience 
that  gives  us  our  sense  of  continuous  individuality.  We  are  con- 
stantly reacting  to  stimuli  on  the  basis  of  our  attitudes.  We  are 
continually  enjoying  or  suffering  the  consequences  of  our  judg- 
ments and  actions.  And  it  is  by  means  of  the  constellation  of  our 
attitudes  that  we  experience  and  react  to  the  welter  of  external 
stimulation  in  a  more  or  less  consistent  way,  unlike  children  lack- 
ing stable  ego-attitudes.  The  consistency  thus  established  defines 
our  role  in  a  more  or  less  lasting  way  in  relation  to  the  persons, 
situations,  and  institutions  we  face.  It  sets  the  boundaries  of  our 
personal  psychological  world,  a  world  in  which  our  egos  act  as  a 
major  reference  point.  The  physicist,  Schrodinger,  after  his  bril- 
liant review  of  modern  developments  in  physics  and  biology  and 
the  implications  they  have  for  the  answer  to  the  question  he  poses 
in  his  little  book,  What  Is  Life?,  comes  to  the  conclusion:  "Yet 
each  of  us  has  the  undisputable  impression  that  the  sum  total 
of  his  own  experience  and  memory  forms  a  unit,  quite  distinct 
from  that  of  any  other  person.  He  refers  to  it  as  'I.'"  [16,  90] 

The  particular  glow  of  individuality,  the  sense  of  "me-ness" 
consciously  or  unconsciously  accompanying  experience  with  an 
ego  reference,  assumes  for  the  individual  a  special  and  unique 
value.  In  one  way  or  another  we  all  try  to  feel  good  about  our- 
selves. In  our  various  personal  and  social  relationships,  we  try  to 
place  ourselves  (our  egos)  securely  and  appropriately  in  our  own 
eyes.  We  try  somehow  to  orient  ourselves  in  the  concrete  world 
we  live  in  until  the  "place"  we  think  we  occupy  is  satisfactory  to 
us.  In  this  process  we  do  what  we  can  to  protect  our  egos  against 
any  onslaughts  from  the  external  world  which  we  feel  might  hurt 
us.  If  our  ego  is  injured  we  resort  to  all  kinds  of  rationalizations, 
to  protective  adjustments,  to  selective  modes  of  reasoning  and 


EGO-STRIVING  IS  NOT  INSTINCTIVE:  NO  INNATE  EGO  DRIVES        101 

behavior  in  which  we  manipulate  things,  persons,  memories,  or 
ideas,  in  a  highly  selective  way. 

These  strivings  related  to  our  ego  assume  very  different  direc- 
tions, proportions,  and  intensities  for  different  individuals,  not  only 
in  different  cultures,  but  in  the  same  culture  or  the  same  small 
group.  And,  furthermore,  the  ego-striving  of  any  given  person 
can  and  does  assume  very  different  intensities  and  proportions  at 
different  times,  depending  upon  the  nature  of  the  situation  in 
which  the  individual  finds  himself  and  upon  internal  physiological 
or  psychological  processes. 

EGO-STRIVING  Is  NOT  INSTINCTIVE:  No  INNATE  EGO  DRIVES 

The  constellation  of  ego-attitudes  which  may  be  referred  to  as 
the  ego  of  the  individual  is  a  genetic  formation  (ch.  7).  But  this 
fact  does  not  mean  that  ego-attitudes  are  mere  cognitive  forma- 
tions devoid  of  strong  affective  or  purposive  properties  prompting 
the  individual  to  diverse  strivings.  As  we  shall  see  in  the  relevant 
studies  on  child  and  adolescent  behavior,  on  group  and  gang  for- 
mations, in  the  experimental  laboratory  as  well  as  in  concrete 
situations  of  everyday  life,  the  individual  strives  in  one  way  or 
another  to  maintain  or  to  enhance  his  ego.  And  in  order  to  learn 
something  about  these  unmistakable  signs  of  striving  related  to 
the  ego  and  to  take  them  into  account  (as  we  must)  in  any  ade- 
quate and  scientific  formulation  of  psychology  we  cannot  resort  to 
the  easy  solution  of  positing  some  innate  ego  drive,  ego  instincts, 
or  ego  needs.  We  have  already  pointed  out  that  what  we  regard 
as  primary  drives  (instincts  or  needs)  are  those  which  have  some 
definite  and  localizable  anatomical  or  physiological  place  in  the 
organism.  Such  instincts  or  drives,  which,  like  hunger,  appear  at 
birth  or,  like  sex,  appear  at  a  later  stage  of  organic  development, 
are  innate.  In  this  sense,  therefore,  any  position  which  implies 
that  there  is  an  ego  drive  comparable  to  other  primary  drives  is 
quite  untenable.  [17, 184f.]  Since  it  is  so  important  that  the  psy- 
chology of  ego-involvements  be  clearly  divorced  from  any  meta- 
physical or  spiritualistic  interpretation,  we  shall  list  here  a  few  of 
the  reasons  for  our  conclusion  that  there  is  no  primary  ego  drive. 

1.  Ego-striving  in  what  seems  to  us  the  only  justifiable  use  of 


102  THE  PROBLEM  OF  EGO-INVOLVEMENTS 

the  term  is  possible  only  after  an  individual  has  acquired  (learned) 
the  complex  and  variously  differentiated  patterns  of  human 
(social)  relationships  that  surround  him.  The  process  involved 
in  grasping  these  relationships  is  essentially  coincident  with  that 
of  ego  development.  Prior  to  the  delineation  of  the  baby's  body 
from  his  surroundings  and  the  formation  of  other  ego-attitudes, 
ego-involvements  cannot  occur.  For  example,  social  distances 
(which  can  be  expressed  in  terms  of  psychological  variables  as 
ego  distances),  social  prejudices  toward  human  groups  and  their 
individual  members  are  found  to  operate  only  after  a  certain  de- 
gree of  ego  development,  that  is,  the  degree  that  makes  it  possible 
for  the  child  to  experience  established  reciprocal  group  relation- 
ships such  as  "we"  and  "they." 

Evidence  that  complex  social  relationships,  along  with  appropri- 
ate values  and  norms  of  experience  and  behavior,  are  not  innately 
experienced  but  are  learned  is  seen  from  the  fact  that  these  rela- 
tionships vary  greatly  from  society  to  society  and  from  time  to 
time  within  a  society.  Even  that  important  range  of  intimate 
personal  relationships  prescribed  by  kinship  constellations  does  not 
always  fit  into  the  biological  proximity  of  consanguinity.  It  even 
varies  widely  within  the  ranges  of  consanguineous  relationships. 
In  fact,  the  relative  proximity  of  the  ego  links  represented  in  kin- 
ship relationships,  in  spite  of  their  biologically  workable  lineage 
based  on  consanguinity,  can  serve  as  a  crucial  piece  of  evidence  that 
ego  links  are  not  innately  experienced  ties. 

Kinship,  family  ties,  the  relative  proximity  of  relatives,  the 
affective  components,  loyalties,  and  responsibilities  attached  to 
them  change  with  variations  in  the  social  organizations  of  different 
societies  as  determined  by  the  character  of  their  socioeconomic 
level  of  development  and  functioning.  Here  we  can  cite  only  a 
few  examples.  The  thesis  is  well  developed  by  such  substantial 
authorities  as  Morgan  [12],  Engels  [2],  Rivers  [15]  and  others. 
A  person  who  cannot  conceive  of  any  other  kinship  relationships 
than  the  ones  he  is  brought  up  to  experience  may  be  surprised  to 
find  such  diverse  types  of  kinship  systems  and  to  learn  from  a 
brief  general  discussion  such  as  Lowie's  that  in  some  societies 
kindred  may  be  "grouped,  for  example,  according  to  generation 


EGO-STRIVING  IS  NOT  INSTINCTIVE:  NO  INNATE  EGO  DRIVES       103 

or  age  level  as  well  as  to  differences  between  direct  and  collateral 
lines  of  descent."  [9,  568] 

For  the  last  80  years  anthropologists  have  piled  up  many  ex- 
amples of  variations  in  kinship  relationships.  In  different  cultures 
and  at  different  periods  in  the  same  cultures,  kinship  relationships 
present  such  a  diverse  and  complex  picture  that  many  an  anthro- 
pologist has  attempted  to  formulate  intricate  classificatory  schemes 
with  the  aim  of  presenting  them  in  some  sort  of  order.  Such 
attempts  were  found  necessary,  for,  as  Radcliffe-Brown  [14] 
points  out, 

If  you  will  take  time  to  study  two  or  three  hundred  kinship  systems 
from  all  parts  of  the  world  you  will  be  impressed,  I  think,  by  the 
great  diversity  that  they  exhibit.  [14,  17] 

A  few  concluding  statements  illustrated  by  examples  from  an 
anthropologist  will  clarify  our  point.  Radcliffe-Brown  notes  the 
various  ranges  of  kinship  relationships  and  their  psychological 
implications  in  terms  of  rights  and  duties: 

A  most  important  character  of  a  kinship  system  is  its  range.  In  a 
narrow  range  system,  such  as  the  English  system  of  the  present  days, 
only  a  limited  number  of  relatives  are  recognized  as  such  in  any  way 
that  entails  any  special  behaviour  or  any  specific  rights  and  duties.  In 
ancient  times  in  England  the  range  was  wider,  since  a  fifth  cousin 
had  a  claim  to  a  share  of  wergild  when  a  man  was  killed.  In  systems 
of  very  wide  range,  such  as  are  found  in  some  non-European  societies, 
a  man  may  recognize  many  hundreds  of  relatives,  toward  each  of 
whom  his  behaviour  is  qualified  by  the  existence  of  the  relation- 
ship. [14, 2] 

And  again: 

A  kinship  system  also  includes  the  existence  of  definite  social  groups. 
The  first  of  these  is  the  domestic  family,  which  is  a  group  of  persons 
who  at  a  particular  time  are  living  together  in  one  dwelling,  or  collec- 
tion of  dwellings,  with  some  sort  of  economic  arrangement  that  we 
may  call  joint  housekeeping.  There  are  many  varieties  of  the  domestic 
family,  varying  in  their  form,  their  size,  and  the  manner  of  their 
common  life.  A  domestic  family  may  consist  of  a  single  elementary 
family,  or  it  may  be  a  group  including  a  hundred  or  more  persons, 
such  as  the  zadruga  of  the  Southern  Slavs,  or  the  taravad  of  the  Nayar. 


104  THE  PROBLEM  OF  EGO-INVOLVEMENTS 

Important  in  some  societies  is  what  may  be  called  a  local  cluster  of 
domestic  families.  In  many  kinship  systems  unilinear  groups  of 
kindred-lineage  groups,  clans  and  moieties  play  an  important  part.  By 
a  kinship  system,  then,  I  mean  a  network  of  social  relations  of  the  kind 
just  defined,  which  thus  constitute  part  of  that  total  network  of  social 
relations  that  I  call  social  structure.  The  rights  and  duties  of  relatives 
to  one  another  and  the  social  usages  that  they  observe  in  their  social 
contacts,  since  it  is  by  these  that  the  relations  are  described,  are  part 
of  the  system.  [14,  2] 

Such  variations  in  kinship  systems  and  family  relationships  are 
not  peculiarities  of  geographically  distant  cultures  alone.  Changes 
in  kinship  and  family  relationships  have  come  with  socioeconomic 
changes  and  changes  reached  in  the  level  of  development.  As  we 
have  seen  previously,  Radcliffe-Brown  cites  a  concrete  case  of  the 
changes  brought  about  historically  in  the  British  kinship  system. 
From  the  same  European  area,  he  gives  the  case  of  a  kinship  system 
which  is  contrary  to  that  of  the  British  system: 

In  Montenegro,  on  the  contrary,  to  take  another  European  system, 
the  father's  brothers  constitute  one  category  and  the  mother's  brothers 
another.  These  relatives  are  distinguished  by  different  terms,  and  so 
are  their  respective  wives,  and  the  social  relations  in  which  a  man 
stands  to  his  two  kinds  of  uncles  show  marked  differences.  [14,  7] 

Stern  [18]  has  recently  made  a  concise  and  intensive  review  of 
the  changes  of  the  status  of  women  which  took  place  in  the  family 
and  in  life  in  general  as  a  consequence  of  modern  technological 
and  social  changes.  For  example,  the  17th  century  wife's  concep- 
tion of  herself  as  given  in  the  following  quotation  is  quite  different 
from  a  modern  New  York  wife's  conception  of  herself: 

The  dutie  of  the  husband  is  to  travel  abroad  to  seeke  living:  and 
the  wives  dutie  is  to  keepe  the  house.  The  dutie  of  the  husband  is  to 
get  money  and  provision;  and  the  wives,  not  vainly  to  spend  it.  The 
dutie  of  the  husband  is  to  deale  with  many  men :  and  of  the  wives,  to 
talke  with  few.  The  dutie  of  the  husband  is,  to  be  entermedling:  and 
of  the  wife,  to  be  solitairie  and  withdrawne.  The  dutie  of  the  man  is, 
to  be  skilfull  in  talke:  and  of  the  wife,  to  boast  of  silence.  The  dutie 
of  the  husband  is,  to  be  a  giver:  and  of  the  wife,  to  be  a  saver  ,  .  . 


EGO-STRIVING  IS  NOT  INSTINCTIVE:  NQ  INNATE  EGO  DRIVES        105 

Now  where  the  husband  and  wife  performed!  the  duties  in  their 
house  we  may  call  it  College  of  Qyietness:  the  house  wherein  they 
are  neglected  we  may  term  it  a  hell.4 

2.  Analysis  of  different  cultures  and  different  social  systems,  or, 
in  some  social  systems,  the  analysis  of  different  groups  within  that 
social  system,  reveals  that  the  ego-striving  of  individuals  may  be 
directed  to  diametrically  opposite  ends  and  goals.  Some  Christian 
saints  strove  to  satisfy  their  egos  by  dispensing  with  all  material 
things,  a  sharp  contrast  to  the  behavior  of  some  "acquisitively 
minded"  individuals  in  most  contemporary  bourgeois  societies. 
People  in  some  cultures  seek  ego-gratifications  through  co-opera- 
tive, relatively  submissive  behavior,  whereas  people  in  other  so- 
cieties obtain  their  gratifications  through  competitive  relatively 
ascendant  behavior.  The  ego-satisfaction  obtained  by  a  Stahkanov- 
ite  in  socialist  competition  is  of  a  far  different  sort  in  its  significance 
to  the  individual  from  the  type  of  satisfaction  it  is  possible  for  a 
skilled  worker  to  obtain  under  capitalist  competition  (ch.  11). 

Likewise,  statuses  aspired  to  by  individuals  change  according  to 
different  hierarchical  status  organizations  in  societies.  As  Linton 
[7]  puts  it: 

In  all  societies  certain  things  are  selected  as  reference  points  for  the 
ascription  of  status.  [7, 115] 

Consequently,  with  shifts  of  reference  points  of  status  in  different 
societies  and  with  major  changes  in  the  same  society  at  different 
times,  the  directions  of  strivings  for  status  shift.  The  fact  that 
reference  points  of  status  do  shift  is  particularly  important  for  the 
psychology  of  ego-attitudes  which  are  the  psychological  counter- 
part of  social  status. 

Similarly,  the  co-operative  and  competitive  behavior  shown  by 
different  personalities  are  indications  of  nothing  other  than  the 
inculcation  of  appropriate  ego-attitudes.  These  contrasting  forms 
of  behavior  are  culturally  determined  and  are  not  the  marks  of 
innate  ego  drives  of  assertiveness  or  submissiveness.  For  example, 
in  a  series  of  studies  representing  different  cultures  from  widely 
scattered  geographical  areas,  it  has  been  impressively  demonstrated 
that  individuals  show  predominately  competitive,  or  co-operative, 

4  Stern  quotes  this  passage  from  [10,  #/?/.]. 


106  THE  PROBLEM  OF  EGO-INVOLVEMENTS 

or  individualistic  behavior  which  is  in  harmony  with  the  behavior 
characteristics  commended  in  their  respective  cultures.  [11]  Thus, 
individuals  in  Manus,  Kwakiutl,  Ifugao  societies  tend  to  exhibit 
rather  competitive  behavior;  whereas  individuals  of  Iroquois, 
Samoa,  Zuni,  Bathonga,  Dakota,  and  Maori  societies  tend  to  show 
rather  co-operative  behavior— all  following  the  characteristics  on 
which  the  prevailing  norms  of  their  respective  societies  put  a 
premium. 

3.  Certainly  there  are  individual  differences  in  the  degree  and 
intensity  of  ego-strivings.  These  individual  differences,  in  our 
opinion,  can  be  accounted  for  by  a  complex  of  factors  such  as 
primary  drives  for  food,  sex,  and  the  like;  individual  bodily  condi- 
tions; individual  differences  in  glandular  functioning;  tempera- 
ment; ability;  and  other  variations  that  may  be  largely  hereditary; 
together  with  the  direction  these  factors  will  take  and  the  signifi- 
cance they  will  have  for  the  individual  within  the  frameworJ^  of 
the  attitudes  acquired  from  his  particular  constellation  of  social 
relationships.  This  fact  is  clearly  brought  out  by  the  biologist 
Julian  Huxley  who  writes: 

Let  us  remind  ourselves  that  superposed  upon  this  purely  biolog- 
ical or  genetic  variability  is  the  even  greater  amount  of  variability  due 
to  differences  of  upbringing,  profession,  and  personal  tastes.  The  final 
result  is  a  degree  of  variation  that  would  be  staggering  if  it  were  not 
so  familiar.  It  would  be  fair  to  say  that,  in  respect  to  mind  and  out- 
look, individual  human  beings  are  separated  by  differences  as  profound 
as  those  which  distinguish  the  major  groups  of  the  animal  kingdom. 
[4,  8,  italics  ours] 

It  should  be  noted  again  in  passing  that  the  constellation  of  these 
factors  may  be  and  usually  is  quite  different  for  different  individ- 
uals and  that  it  will  vary  within  the  same  individual  with  biological 
changes  (for  example,  those  that  come  about  with  increasing  age), 
with  the  relative  satisfaction  of  his  basic  needs,  and  with  the 
opportunity  afforded  by  the  situation  or  the  environment  for  the 
maintenance  or  enhancement  of  his  position.  Sudden  and  appar- 
ently inconsistent  shifts  in  the  direction  a  person's  efforts  will  take 
can  become  meaningful  only  when  these  pursuits,  these  derived 


EGO-STRIVING  IS  NOT  INSTINCTIVE:  NO  INNATE  EGO  DRIVES        107 

drives,  are  related  to  learned  ego-involved  values.  [1,  41-52] 
Chapter  10,  dealing  with  ego-involvements  in  group  situations,  is 
filled  with  data  that  point  to  this  conclusion. 

4.  Subnormal  individuals,  such  as  imbeciles,  have  ego  structures 
that  can  be  demonstrated  to  be  extremely  narrow  and  confined. 
To  borrow  a  phrase  used  by  Huxley  in  another  connection,  the 
difference  between  such  a  subnormal  person  as  might  be  found  in 
almost  any  state  institution  for  the  feeble-minded  and  an  intelli- 
gent adult  vigorously  trying  to  achieve  certain  desired  ends,  is 
"comparable  in  extent  with  that  between  a  sponge  and  a  higher 
mammal."  [4,  8]    However,  both  the  subnormal  individual  we 
might  select  at  random  and  an  intelligent  individual  displaying  a 
continuous  and  complex  ego-striving  would  more  than  likely  be 
found  to  have  similar  good  appetites  for  food,  similar  needs  for 
sexual  satisfactions,  and  so  on. 

5.  Under  certain  conditions,  either  physiological,  pathological, 
or  situational,  the  ego  may  break  down,  dissolve,  apparently  dis- 
appear for  either  a  shorter  or  a  longer  time.    This  is  not  true  of 
innate  or  biologically  given  instincts  or  drives  which,  by  almost 
any  definition,  are  constantly,  and  recurrently,  or  periodically 
manifested.   We  discuss  later  in  some  detail  instances  of  ego-break- 
down, ranging  from  the  ungentlemanly  behavior  of  individuals 
when  they  are  drunk,  the  unladylike  behavior  of  socially  refined 
women  when  they  are  desperate  for  food,  the  resort  to  prostitutes 
of  men  in  love  when  their  sex  needs  become  extreme,  to  more 
complicated  and  enduring  dissociations  brought  about  by  extreme 
prolonged  situations  or  by  pathological  conditions  (ch.  12).    In  all 
these  cases  established  ego-attitudes  become  separated  from  judg- 
ment and  behavior.    A  person  does  not  act  like  "his  normal  self." 

6.  We  should  include  in  our  list  the  point  previously  emphasized 
that  there  is  no  evidence  for  the  existence  of  an  ego  drive  in  the 
sense  that  this  drive,  like  primary  drives  for  food  and  sex,  has  a 
definitely  assignable  locus  in  the  individual  organism.    We  are,  of 
course,  not  at  all  denying  the  fact  that  psychological  phenomena 
and  experiences,  such  as  ego-involvements,  are  due  to  complex 
chemical  or  physical  reactions  within  the  organism.    As  Schro- 
dinger  says: 


108  THE  PROBLEM  OF  EGO-INVOLVEMENTS 

My  body  functions  as  a  pure  mechanism  according  to  the  Laws  of 
Nature.  [16, 87] 

It  is  not  at  all  impossible  or  improbable  that  in  the  unforeseeable 
future  the  physical  scientist  may  work  out  some  intricate  and  valid 
formulas  to  describe  those  processes  in  the  organism  that  are  expe- 
rienced as  ego-involvements.  But  if  and  when  this  is  accomplished, 
the  qualitative  differences  between  such  formulations  and  our 
current  relatively  precise  knowledge  concerning  the  physical  basis 
of  primary  drives  such  as  hunger  and  sex  will  almost  surely  be  so 
vast  that  they  cannot  possibly  be  classified  together. 

7.  There  is  no  ego  formation,  no  ego-involvement,  in  animals 
in  the  sense  of  reciprocal  interpersonal  relationships  and  group 
relationships  on  a  conceptual  level.  There  is  no  animal  culture 
comparable  to  that  of  man.  Ego  formation  and  ego-involvement 
require  that  the  organism  shall  be  capable  of  functioning  on  a 
conceptual  or  symbolic  level.  The  values,  standards,  or  norms  of 
social  relationship  to  which  the  human  being  relates  and  identifies 
himself  are  possible  only  because  he  is  able  to  grasp  these  values, 
standards,  or  norms  conceptually.  There  can  obviously  be  no 
involvement  or  identification  with  let  us  say,  class,  nationality,  or 
occupational  groups,  unless  the  meaning  and  significance  of  a 
"class,"  a  "nation,"  or  an  "occupation"  can  be  conceived  on  a  con- 
ceptual level.  And  in  turn,  of  course,  social  groupings,  organiza- 
tions and  institutions  characteristic  of  human  society  would  be 
impossible  without  the  interconnections,  loyalties,  and  traditions 
that  are  based  on  conceptual  thinking. 

This  fact  is  of  the  utmost  importance.  For  it  means  that  find- 
ings concerned  with  social  behavior  on  the  subhuman  level  may 
by  no  means  be  applicable  to  the  human  level  when  reciprocal 
human  relationships  or  the  conceptual  level  of  human  interaction 
is  in  question.  It  means  that  in  the  psychology  of  ego-involve- 
ments we  are  dealing  with  a  unique  human  characteristic,  an 
emergent  that  is  qualitatively  different  from  other  evolutionary 
products,  an  emergent  that  follows  the  dialectic  process.  What 
we  observe  as  animal  aggressiveness  and  animal  affective  fixations 
are  determined  by  momentary  situations,  by  relative  physical 
strength,  by  brute  force,  or  by  fixations  built  up  through  the 


EGO-STRIVING  IS  NOT  INSTINCTIVE:  NO  INNATE  EGO  DRIVES        109 

process  of  conditioning  which  docs  not  involve  reciprocal  relation- 
ships on  the  conceptual  level. 

The  uniqueness  of  human  culture  and  its  dependence  upon 
language  (concept)  formation  are  nicely  brought  out  and  well- 
documented  by  Warden,  an  outstanding  comparative  psychologist, 
in  his  The  Emergence  of  Human  Culture.  [21]  He  writes  that 

.  .  .  the  simple  truth  seems  to  be  that  man's  primary  claim  to 
distinction  rests  upon  the  fact  that  he  alone  possesses  a  genuine  cul- 
ture. [21,  3] 

The  transition  from  the  humanoid  level  to  the  first  human  type  was 
coincident  with  the  emergence  of  the  cultural  order.  [21,  99] 

After  surveying  the  social  life  of  insects,  birds,  and  mammals,  he 
states  bluntly  that  "even  the  rudiments  of  culture  do  not  exist 
among  sub-human  organisms."  [65] 
Our  human  culture,  then,  is  "an  emergent,"  [23]  and 

An  emergent  system  has  new  properties  and  new  modes  of  organiza- 
tion that  seem  to  bear  no  definite  relation  to  the  old  order  from  which 
it  arose.  [21,  24] 

In  describing  this  emergent,  human  culture,  Warden  points  out 
that 

This  new  type  of  social  integration  was  unique  in  that  it  reached 
above  the  purely  biosocial  level.  For  the  first  time,  invention,  effective 
communication  within  the  group,  and  the  social  habituation  of  the 
young  became  possible.  The  activities  of  the  individual  were  no  longer 
limited  to  the  instinctive  repertory  of  the  species.  New  skills  could  be 
invented  by  the  superior  individual  and  passed  along  to  the  other 
members  of  the  group  by  means  of  language.  Simple  folkways  could 
now  develop  out  of  the  greater  variety  and  intimacy  of  social  contacts. 
These  new  ways  of  doing  and  acting,  as  accepted  by  the  group,  could 
be  impressed  upon  the  young  through  language  and  imitation.  Such 
of  the  skills  and  folkways  as  survived  from  generation  to  generation 
comprised  a  rude  tradition.  This  new  mode  of  social  integration  was 
cultural  rather  than  biosocial.  [21, 105  /.] 

Throughout  his  book,  Warden  emphasizes  the  fact  that 

The  cultural  order  was  unique  because  the  presence  of  language 
made  possible  a  new  and  most  important  type  of  social  integration. 
[108] 


110  THE  PROBLEM  OF  EGO-INVOLVEMENTS 

Communication,  invention,  and  social  habituation  he  regards  as 
the  three  "basic  mechanisms"  that  make  the  human  cultural  order 
possible.  [21]  Concerning  communication  he  states  that 

It  seems  altogether  unlikely  that  a  culture  could  emerge  and  main- 
tain itself  without  the  support  of  a  well-developed  capacity  for  vocal 
language.  [22]  ...  Language  ^becomes  an  important  instrument  for 
the  broadening  of  cultural  contacts.  The  child  now  learns  to  express 
its  own  desires  effectively  and  to  understand  and  heed  those  of  others. 
It  learns  to  recognize  such  social  distinctions  as  attach  to  age,  rank, 
and  other  tribal  relationships.  It  comes  to  play  games  according  to 
rule  and  custom.  The  channels  of  thought  are  marked  out  by  the 
folklore  and  ideas  of  current  conversation.  The  attitudes  and  beliefs 
of  the  group  gradually  become  those  of  the  child.  [7/.J  .  .  .  Moral 
sentiments  are  inculcated  and  many  additional  duties  and  obligations 
are  imposed.  Conformity  with  group  norms  of  conduct  takes  on  a 
new  importance  because  of  its  relation  to  personal  success  and  pres- 
tige.  [21,  8] 

The  point  has  also  been  fully  recognized  by  outstanding  biolo- 
gists of  our  day.  For  example,  Julian  Huxley,  in  a  chapter,  "The 
Uniqueness  of  Man,"  in  his  Man  Stands  Alone  [4]  writes: 

...  let  us  remind  ourselves  that  the  gap  between  human  and  animal 
thought  is  much  greater  than  is  usually  supposed.  The  tendency  to 
project  familiar  human  qualities  into  animals  is  very  strong,  and  col- 
ours the  ideas  of  nearly  all  people  who  have  not  special  familiarity 
both  with  animal  behavior  and  scientific  method.  [19]  .  .  .  In  point 
of  fact,  the  great  majority  of  man's  activities  and  characteristics  are 
by-products  of  his  primary  distinctive  characteristics,  and  therefore, 
life  them,  biologically  unique.  On  the  one  hand,  conversation,  or- 
ganized games,  education,  sport,  paid  work,  gardening,  the  theatre; 
on  the  other,  conscience,  duty,  sin,  humiliation,  vice,  penitence — these 
are  all  such  unique  by-products.  The  trouble,  indeed,  is  to  find  any 
human  activities  which  are  not  unique.  Even  the  fundamental  bio- 
logical attributes  such  as  eating,  sleeping,  and  mating  have  been  tricked 
out  by  man  with  all  \inds  of  unique  frills  and  peculiarities.  [29,  italics 
ours] 

The  first  and  most  obviously  unique  characteristic  of  man  is  his 
capacity  for  conceptual  thought;  if  you  prefer  objective  terms,  you  will 
say  his  employment  of  true  speech,  but  that  is  only  another  way  of 
saying  the  same  thing.  True  speech  involves  the  use  o£  verbal  signs 


EGO-STRIVING  IS  NOT  INSTINCTIVE:  NO  INNATE  EGO  DRIVES        111 

for  objects,  not  merely  for  feelings.  Plenty  of  animals  can  express  the 
fact  that  they  are  hungry;  but  none  except  man  can  ask  for  an  egg  or 
a  banana.  [3,  italics  ours] 

Words  are  tools  which  automatically  carve  concepts  out  of  experi- 
ence. The  faculty  of  recognizing  objects  as  members  of  a  class  pro- 
vides the  potential  basis  for  the  concept;  the  use  of  words  at  once 
actualizes  the  potentiality.  This  basic  human  property  has  had  many 
consequences.  The  most  important  was  the  development  of  a  cumu- 
lative tradition.  The  beginnings  of  tradition,  by  which  experience  is 
transmitted  from  one  generation  to  the  next,  are  to  be  seen  in  many 
higher  animals.  But  in  no  case  is  the  tradition  cumulative.  Offspring 
learn  from  parents,  but  they  learn  the  same  kind  and  quantity  of 
lessons  as  they,  in  turn,  impart:  the  transmission  of  experience  never 
bridges  more  than  one  generation.  In  man,  however,  tradition  is  an 
independent  and  potentially  permanent  activity,  capable  of  indefinite 
improvement  in  quality  and  increase  in  quantity.  It  constitutes  a  new 
accessory  process  of  heredity  in  evolution,  running  side  by  side  with 
the  biological  process,  a  heredity  of  experience  to  supplement  the  uni- 
versal heredity  of  living  substance.  The  existence  of  a  cumulative 
tradition  has  as  its  chief  consequence — or  if  you  prefer,  its  chief 
objective  manifestation — the  progressive  improvement  of  human  tools 
and  machinery.  Many  animals  employ  tools;  but  they  are  always 
crude  tools  employed  in  a  crude  way.  Elaborate  tools  and  skilled 
technique  can  develop  only  with  the  aid  of  speech  and  tradition.  [3f.] 
.  .  .  Speech,  tradition,  and  tools  have  led  to  many  other  unique  prop- 
erties of  man.  These  are,  for  the  most  part,  obvious  and  well  known, 
and  I  propose  to  leave  them  aside  until  I  have  dealt  with  some  less 
familiar  human  characteristics.  [5] 

The  essential  character  of  man  as  a  dominant  organism  is  con~ 
ceptual  thought.  And  conceptual  thought  could  have  arisen  only  in  a 
multicellular  animal,  an  animal  with  bilateral  symmetry,  head  and 
blood  system,  a  vertebrate  as  against  a  mollusc  or  an  arthropod,  a 
land  vertebrate  among  vertebrates,  a  mammal  among  land  vertebrates. 
Finally,  it  could  have  arisen  only  in  a  mammalian  line  which  was 
gregarious,  which  produced  one  young  at  birth  instead  of  several,  and 
which  had  recently  become  terrestrial  after  a  long  period  of  arboreal 
life.  There  is  only  one  group  of  animals  which  fulfils  these  conditions 
— a  terrestrial  offshoot  of  the  higher  Primates.  Thus  not  merely  has 
conceptual  thought  been  evolved  only  in  man:  it  could  not  have  been 
evolved  except  in  man.  There  is  but  one  path  of  unlimited  progress 
through  the  evolutionary  maze.  The  course  of  human  evolution  is  as 


112  THE  PROBLEM  OF  EGO-INVOLVEMENTS 

unique  as  its  result.  It  is  unique  not  in  the  trivial  sense  of  being  a 
different  course  from  that  of  any  other  organism,  but  in  the  pro- 
founder  sense  of  being  the  only  path  that  could  have  achieved  the 
essential  characters  of  man.  Conceptual  thought  on  this  planet  is 
inevitably  associated  with  a  particular  type  of  Primate  body  and 
Primate  brain.  [4, 15  /.,  italics  ours] 

Another  biologist,  Leo  Loeb,  writing  in  1945  on  the  Biological 
Basis  of  Individuality  [8]  shows  the  necessity  of  concept  forma- 
tion for  man's  unique  individuality.  He  uses  this  fact  as  a  stepping 
stone  for  his  own  formulation,  in  his  own  terminology,  of  the 
way  in  which  the  ego  (the  "I")  emerges  in  man  as  a  distinctive 
characteristic.  And  he  goes  still  further  in  his  book  to  indicate 
the  implications  this  emergence  has  for  man's  identifications  and 
social  relationships. 

According  to  Loeb,  the  ability  to  form  concepts,  to  think  ab- 
stractly and  synthetically,  has  come  about  suddenly  and  appears 
only  with  the  development  of  man. 

There  has  ...  taken  place  an  evolution  of  two  types  of  individu- 
ality. The  first  is  connected  with  the  differentiation  of  the  organ 
differentials  and  with  the  evolution  of  the  individuality  differential 
and  its  manifestations,  from  a  very  primitive  character  to  the  state  of 
great  refinement  reached  in  mammals.  The  second  is  connected  with 
the  evolution  of  the  psychical-social  factors,  leading  to  the  gradual 
creation  and  refinement  of  the  individual  in  the  psychical-social  sense. 
This  second  evolutionary  process  is  related  only  indirectly  to  the 
development  of  the  individuality  differentials;  it  depends  directly  upon 
the  increasing  complexity  and  refinement  of  certain  organ  differentials, 
especially  of  the  nervous  system.  There  is,  therefore,  no  perfect 
parallelism  between  these  two  evolutionary  processes.  While  in  the 
first  process  a  gradual,  step-by-step  development  of  the  individuality 
differential  occurs,  in  the  second  process  the  most  important,  jar- 
reaching  change  has  ta\en  place  suddenly  in  the  transition  from  an- 
thropoid apes  to  man.  [8,  654,  italics  ours] 

He  finds  no  evidence  of  these  higher  thought  processes  even  in 
anthropoid  apes. 

Proceeding  now  from  the  other  higher  mammals  to  man,  very  pro- 
nounced complications  in  the  modes  of  reactions  are  observed.  Not 
only  does  the  environment,  which  acts  on  us  through  our  sense  organs, 


EGO-STRIVING  IS  NOT  INSTINCTIVE:  NO  INNATE  EGO  DRIVES       113 

induce  changes  which  have  a  much  more  varied  and  also  more  lasting 
effect  on  our  behavior  than  in  other  mammals,  but  abstraction  and 
synthesis,  in  which  the  elements  in  the  environment  are  separated  and 
then  re-arranged  in  new  combinations,  become  very  prominent. 
Thoughts  develop,  in  which  the  constituents  of  the  environment  may 
appear  in  combinations  different  from  those  in  which  they  occur  under 
natural  conditions;  through  shifting  of  these  constituents  new  concepts 
are  formed.  [8,  619] 

As  a  consequence  of  man's  unique  mental  capacities  and  his 
interaction  with  both  the  natural  and  social  environment,  the  "I" 
concept  is  developed. 

As  a  result  partly  of  rational  thought,  but  largely  also  because  of 
the  friction,  antagonism  and  pain  developing  in  the  social  and  natural 
struggle,  the  concept  of  the  "I"  as  contrasted  with  the  concept  of 
others  and  of  the  surrounding  world,  develops.  The  "I"  is  the  indi- 
viduality in  the  psychical-social  sense.  [8,  620,  italics  ours] 

8.  The  changes  in  ego  relationships  that  result  from  lobotomy 
and  lobectomy  demonstrate  in  a  crucial  way  the  close  functional 
relationship  between  ego  formation  and  ego  functioning  on  the 
one  hand,  and,  on  the  other  hand,  psychological  functioning  on 
a  conceptual  level.  Further  evidence  of  the  dependence  of  ego 
formation  on  psychological  functioning  at  the  conceptual  level  is 
found  in  observations  of  persons  who  have  lost  both  their  ego 
relationships  and  their  abstract  (conceptual)  functioning  follow- 
ing certain  injuries  to  the  frontal  lobes  whose  tremendous  devel- 
opment in  the  human  species  gives  man  his  unique  ability  to 
function  psychologically  on  an  abstract  conceptual  level.  Evi- 
dence concerning  these  two  points  is  found  later  in  Chapter  12 
on  pages  425  ff.  and  431  ff. 

How  to  account  for  ego-striving.  If  ego-strivings  do  not  have 
an  instinctive  basis  in  the  sense  of  hunger  and  sex  needs  or  drives, 
how  are  psychologists  to  account  for  these  ego-strivings  with  all 
their  rich  affective  halo,  their  emotional  warmth,  and  their  vary- 
ing intensity?  As  we  indicated  in  the  introduction,  a  detailed 
account  of  motivation  as  it  relates  to  social  psychology  cannot  be 
treated  here.  And  the  more  precise  role  of  ego-striving  in  the 
whole  network  of  instinctive  behavior,  reflexes,  habits,  primary 


114  THE  PROBLEM  OF  EGO-INVOLVEMENTS 

and  derived  drives,  personality  traits,  frustrations,  and  the  like, 
is  being  deliberately  reserved  for  later  discussion.  In  this  book  we 
are  concerned  chiefly  with  the  place  of  the  ego  in  systematic  social 
psychology. 

We  have  already  indicated  the  main  outline  of  our  position. 
We  can  summarize  it  briefly  again  here.  The  evidence  we  have 
already  presented,  together  with  that  to  follow,  gives  us  some- 
what the  following  picture:  our  major  psychological  activities 
— our  perception,  judgment,  remembering,  and  so  on — take  place 
in  referential  frameworks.  The  ego  is  no  exception  to  this  general 
rule.  We  learn  (or  sometimes  determine)  what  values,  goals, 
standards,  or  norms  are  desirable  for  us.  These  become  incorpo- 
rated as  our  values,  our  goals.  The  referential  framework  of  the 
ego  is  therefore  these  values,  goals,  standards,  or  norms  which  have 
become  our  major  attitudes,  which  have  become  so  large  a  part 
of  what  we  refer  to  as  me.  These  values,  goals,  standards,  or 
norms  which  become  our  attitudes  are  represented  by,  set  by,  or 
created  by  group  activities  and  social  situations  that  form  the  con- 
stellation of  social  relationships  with  which  we  come  in  contact. 
Major  attitudes  are  thus  derived  from  groups  to  which  we  learn 
to  relate  ourselves  or  which  we  regard  ourselves  as  members  of: 
reference  groups,  membership  groups.  My  identification  and 
allegiances,  my  status  and  position  are  determined  with  respect  to 
these  reference  and  membership  groups.  If  I  can  relate  myself 
securely  to  these  reference  groups,  7  feel  secure  in  my  general 
status,  insofar  as  the  values  of  the  reference  groups  themselves  are 
compatible.  If  I  can  anchor  myself  securely  in  a  membership 
group,  /  feel  secure  in  my  more  specific  membership  position. 
Although  we  may  have  a  very  clear  knowledge  of  what  the  norms 
of  the  larger  society  or  of  more  specific  groups  are,  mere  knowl- 
edge of  these  norms  does  not  necessarily  in  itself  induce  identifica- 
tions. When  we  discuss  the  "deviant"  behavior  of  gang  members 
we  shall  find  clear  evidence  that  norms  must  serve  the  function  of 
helping  us  place  ourselves  in  social  relationships,  must  offer  us  the 
possibility  (consciously  or  unconsciously)  of  acquiring,  maintain- 
ing, or  enhancing  our  status  before  they  become  ours  (ch.  10). 

These  reference  and  membership  groups  not  only  set  for  us  the 
values  or  norms  that  become  our  attitudes:  they  further  make  con- 


REFERENCES  115 

stant  demands  on  our  loyalties  as  time  goes  on.  Some  of  these 
loyalties  change  with  age  as  we  shift  our  identifications.  This  will 
be  most  strikingly  brought  out  when  we  discuss  ego  formation  and 
re-formation  in  adolescents  (ch.  9).  Other  loyalties  change  under 
the  impact  of  new  situations  (chs.  10,  11).  But  whoever  a  person 
may  be  and  whatever  social  milieu  surrounds  him,  what  he  learns 
to  regard  as  his  loyalty,  his  purpose,  his  ambition,  his  striving  to 
gratify  basic  needs  or  drives  is  affected,  regulated,  or  directed  in  a 
major  way  by  the  referential  framework  that  has  become  so  large 
a  part  of  him  in  the  course  of  his  social  development.  Ego-striv- 
ing, then,  is  the  individual's  effort  to  place  himself  securely  in 
those  constellations  of  human  relationships  that  represent  for  him 
desirable  values,  that  will  make  his  status  or  position  secure. 

Owing  to  individual  differences  in  characteristics  such  as  tem- 
perament, ability,  or  energy  there  are  enormous  and  important 
differences  in  the  manner,  persistence,  and  intensity  in  which  ego- 
striving  proceeds.  And  the  relative  position  which  will  be  re- 
garded as  personally  satisfying  to  an  individual  is  determined  by 
such  individual  differences,  as  well  as  by  the  specific  values  he  has 
learned.  However,  the  important  fact  for  social  psychologists  to 
remember  is  that  these  individual  differences  all  function  within 
the  framework  of  norms  prescribed  by  the  reference  or  member- 
ship groups.  Equally  healthy,  energetic,  intelligent,  resourceful 
people,  endowed  with  similar  temperamental  characteristics  and 
with  similar  glandular  structure  would  inevitably  manifest  differ- 
ent, perhaps  quite  opposite,  ego-strivings  according  to  the  differing 
or  contrasting  referential  frameworks  in  which  their  strivings  took 
place. 

REFERENCES 

1.  CANTRIL,  H.,  The  Psychology  of  Social  Movements,  New  York:  Wiley,  1941. 

2.  ENGELS,  F.,  The  Origin  of  the  Family,  Private  Property  and  the  State,  New 

York:  International,  1942,  first  published  1884. 

3.  HEALY,  W.,  and  A.  F.  BRONNER,  New  Light  on  Delinquency  and  Its  Treatment, 

New  Haven:  Yale  Univ.  Press,  copyright  1936. 

4.  HUXLEY,  J.,  Man  Stands  Alone,  New  York:  Harper,  copyright  1927. 

5.  KATZAROFF,  D.,  Contribution  a  Petude  de  la  recognition,  Arch,  de  Psych.,  11, 

1911, 1-78. 

6.  KOFFKA,  K.,  Principles  of  Gestalt  Psychology,  New  York:  Harcourt,  grace,  1935. 


116  THE  PROBLEM  OF  EGO-INVOLVEMENTS 

7.  LINTON,  R.,  The  Study  of  Man:  An  Introduction,  New  York:  Applcton-Century, 

copyright  1936,  student's  ed. 

8.  LOEB,  L.,  The  Biological  Basis  of  Individuality,  Springfield:  Thomas,  copyright 

1945. 

9.  LOWIE,  R.  H.,  Kinship,  in  Encyclopaedia  of  the  Social  Sciences,  New  York: 

Macmillan,  1932,  8,  568-72. 

10.  LUMPKIN,  K.  DuP.,  The  Family:  A  Study  of  Member  Roles,  Chapel  Hill: 

Univ.  N.  Carolina  Press,  copyright  1933. 

11.  MEAD,  M.  (ed.),  Cooperation  and.  Competition  among  Primitive  Peoples,  New 

York:  McGraw-Hill,  1937. 

12.  MORGAN,  L.  H.,  Ancient  Society  or  Researches  in  the  Lines  of  Human  Progress 

from  Savagery  through  Barbarism  to  Civilization,  New  York:  Holt,  1877. 

13.  MURPHY,  G.,  L.  B.  MURPHY,  and  T.  NEWCOMB,  Experimental  Social  Psychol- 

ogy, New  York,  Harper,  copyright  1937. 

14.  RADCLIFFE-BROWN,  A.  R.,  The  study  of  kinship  systems,  Presidential  Address, 

/.  Roy.  Anthropol  Inst.  of  Great  Britain  and  Ireland,  1941,  71,  parts  I  and  II. 

15.  RIVERS,  W.  H.  R.,  History  of  Melanesian  Society,  Cambridge,  England:  Univ. 

Press,  1914,  vols.  I  and  EL 

16.  SCHRODINGER,  E.,  What  Is  Life?,  New  York:  Macmillan,  copyright  1945. 

17.  SHERIF,  M.,  The  Psychology  of  Social  Norms,  New  York:  Harper,  1936. 

18.  STERN,  B.  J.,  The  family  and  cultural  change,  Am.  Sociol.  Rev.,  copyright 

1939,  4,  199-208. 

19.  STERN,  W.,  General  Psychology,  Prom  the  Personalistic  Standpoint,  trans,  by 

H.  D.  Spoerl,  New  York:  Macmillan,  1938. 

20.  WALLON,  H.,  Les  Origines  du  caractere  chez  I' enfant,  Paris:  Presses  Universi- 

taires  de  France,  1933. 

21.  WARDEN,  C,  J,,  The  Emergence  of  Human  Culture,  New  York:  Macmillan, 

copyright  1936. 


CHAPTER  6 
EXPERIMENTS  ON  EGO-INVOLVEMENTS 

We  have  said  that  what  an  individual  comes  to  regard  as  himself 
is  a  genetic  development,  a  product  of  learning.  In  the  normal 
course  of  affairs,  the  components  of  the  ego  include  the  individual's 
body  and  physical  characteristics;  the  things  he  learns  belong  to 
him,  such  as  his  clothes,  his  toys,  his  keepsakes,  his  room,  his  hut, 
his  house,  his  mother,  his  sweetheart,  his  children;  together  with 
a  whole  host  of  social  values  he  also  learns  and  with  which  he 
identifies  himself — his  country,  his  politics,  his  language,  his  man- 
ner of  dressing,  the  characteristics  of  his  particular  society. 

In  spite  of  the  relative  similarity  of  the  norms  to  which  an 
individual  in  a  given  society  or  a  group  may  be  exposed,  the  con- 
tent of  any  single  individual's  ego,  what  he  regards  as  himself,  is 
a  rather  distinct  constellation  of  social  and  personal  values  that 
vary  not  only  in  their  number  and  nature  but  also  in  the  intensity 
with  which  they  are  held.  Within  the  range  of  individual  differ- 
ences due  to  variations  in  instinctual  drive,  ability,  and  tempera- 
ment, the  similarity  of  the  content  of  individual  egos  will  increase, 
of  course,  with  the  uniformity  of  the  situations,  experiences.,  or 
norms  to  which  an  individual  is  exposed. 

These  contents  of  the  ego,  these  things,  persons,  ways  of  con- 
ducting oneself,  social  norms  of  various  kinds,  provide  for  the 
individual  the  standards  of  judgment  or  frames  of  reference  which 
determine  to  such  an  important  degree  his  social  behavior  and 
reactions.  And  when  any  stimulus  or  situation  is  consciously  or 
unconsciously  related  to  them  by  the  individual,  we  can  say  there 
is  "ego-involvement."  Thus,  the  ego  in  its  various  capacities  enters 
in  as  an  important  determinant  which  may  color,  modify,  or  alter 
our  experiences  and  behavior  in  almost  any  situation.  For  our 
standards,  our  values,  our  goals  and  ambitions,  our  ways  of  doing 

things  have  become  involved.   We  feel  elated,  restricted,  gratified, 

117 


118  EXPERIMENTS  ON  EGO-INVOLVEMENTS 

supported,  disturbed,  or  insecure,  in  these  ego-involving  situations. 

This  ego-involvement  can  and  does  range  from  what  may  be  a 
temporary  moderate  involvement  in  a  laboratory  experiment  with 
some  task  to  be  performed  where  we  feel  that  somehow  our 
capacities  or  abilities  are  at  stake,  to  complex  social  situations  in 
which  we  feel  involved,  because  of  some  threat  to,  or  enhance- 
ment of,  our  position  as  a  member  of  some  gang,  group,  or  class 
we  identify  ourselves  with. 

In  recent  years  the  fact  of  ego-involvements  has  been  demon- 
strated experimentally  in  the  laboratory  and  in  other  controlled 
investigations  concerned  with  more  everyday  life  situations.  We 
are  citing  some  of  these  studies  here  to  show  that  the  concept  of 
ego-involvement  can  be  and  has  been  successfully  verified  under 
the  rigorous  controls  imposed  by  scientific  method.1 

EGO-INVOLVEMENTS  IN  LABORATORY  TASKS 

In  one  of  his  characteristic  insights  as  a  psychologist,  James 
pointed  out  in  his  Principles  (1890)  that  our  "self -feeling"  is  de- 
termined by  the  "ratio  of  our  actualities  to  our  supposed  poten- 
tialities." [35] 

I,  who  for  the  time  have  staked  my  all  on  being  a  psychologist,  am 
mortified  if  others  know  much  more  psychology  than  I.  But  I  am 
contented  to  wallow  in  the  grossest  ignorance  of  Greek.  My  defi- 
ciencies there  give  me  no  sense  of  personal  humiliation  at  all.  Had  I 
"pretensions"  to  be  a  linguist,  it  would  have  been  just  the  reverse. 
So  we  have  the  paradox  of  a  man  shamed  to  death  because  he  is  only 
the  second  pugilist  or  the  second  oarsman  in  the  world.  That  he  is 
able  to  beat  the  whole  population  of  the  globe  minus  one  is  nothing; 
he  has  "pitted"  himself  to  beat  that  one;  and  as  long  as  he  doesn't  do 
that  nothing  else  counts.  He  is  to  his  own  regard  as  if  he  were  not, 
indeed  he  is  not. 

Yonder  puny  fellow,  however,  whom  every  one  can  beat,  suffers  no 
chagrin  about  it,  for  he  has  long  ago  abandoned  the  attempt  to  "carry 
that  line,"  as  the  merchants  say,  of  self  at  all.  With  no  attempt  there 
can  be  no  failure;  with  no  failure  no  humiliation.  So  our  self-feeling 

1  A  comparatively  early  and  important  recognition  of  the  fact  of  ego-involve- 
ment was  made  by  Murphy,  Murphy,  and  Newcomb  when  in  1937  they  grouped 
a  number  of  different  experiments  together  in  their  discussion  of  the  ego.  [50] 


EGO-INVOLVEMENTS  IN  LABORATORY  TASKS  119 

in  this  world  depends  entirely  on  what  we  bac\  ourselves  to  be  and  do. 
It  is  determined  by  the  ratio  of  our  actualities  to  our  supposed  poten- 
tialities; a  fraction  of  which  our  pretensions  are  the  denominator  and 

the  numerator  our  success:  thus,  Self-esteem  =  - : — .    Such  a 

Pretensions 

fraction  may  be  increased  as  well  by  diminishing  the  denominator  as 
by  increasing  the  numerator.  To  give  up  pretensions  is  as  blessed  a 
relief  as  to  get  them  gratified.  .  .  .  [35,  310  f.] 

Baldwin,  writing  in  1897,  noted  that  a  man's  "opinion  of  others 
must  be  referred  to  the  same  standards  by  which  he  judges  him- 
self" [5,  79],  whereas  Cooley,  in  1902,  acknowledging  his  debt  to 
James  and  Baldwin,  wrote  that 

The  thing  that  moves  us  to  pride  or  shame  is  not  the  mere  mechani- 
cal reflection  of  ourselves,  but  an  imputed  sentiment,  the  imagined 
effect  of  this  reflection  upon  another's  mind.  This  is  evident  from 
the  fact  that  the  character  and  weight  of  that  other,  in  whose  mind 
we  see  ourselves,  makes  all  the  difference  with  our  feeling.  We  are 
ashamed  to  seem  evasive  in  the  presence  of  a  straightforward  man, 
cowardly  in  the  presence  of  a  brave  one,  gross  in  the  eyes  of  a  refined 
one,  and  so  on.  [15, 184] 

In  other  words,  James,  Baldwin,  and  Cooley,  in  the  comparatively 
early  days  of  psychology,  saw  the  referential  nature  of  judgments, 
recognized  that  these  were  anchored  in  the  ego. 

These  early  observations  are  of  more  than  mere  historical  inter- 
est. Contemporary  psychoanalysts  explicitly  relate  their  modern 
formulations  of  neuroses  to  James's  descriptions  of  the  "social  self." 
For  example,  Karen  Horney,  writing  in  1939,  states  that 

To  some  extent  everyone  living  in  an  organized  community  must 
keep  up  appearances.  To  some  extent  every  one  of  us  has  imbibed 
the  standards  of  the  environment.  To  some  extent  we  are  all  depend- 
ent on  the  regard  others  have  for  us.  [32,  216  f.] 

And  she  notes  that 

William  James  has  said  that  to  give  up  pretensions  is  as  blessed  a 
relief  as  to  have  them  gratified;  judging  from  observations  in  analysis 
the  relief  resulting  from  giving  them  up  seems  to  be  the  greater  of 
the  two.  [32,  231] 


120  EXPERIMENTS  ON  EGO-INVOLVEMENTS 

And  the  considerable  experimentation  conducted  in  the  past  15 
years  on  what  has  become  known  as  the  level  of  aspiration  is  given 
a  theoretical  framework  by  these  earlier  writers. 

In  Hoppe's  [31]  experiment  on  the  level  of  aspiration  (1930) 
we  have  a  demonstration  that  levels  of  aspiration  are  essentially 
ego-involving  frames  of  reference  or  anchoring  points.  Success 
and  failure  are  determined  with  respect  to  subjectively  established 
scales:  subjects  adjust  their  level  of  aspiration  according  to  the 
degree  of  success  on  previous  performances;  if  the  level  of  per- 
formance is  outside  the  range  of  the  level  of  aspiration  (either 
above  or  below  it),  there  is  no  experience  of  success  or  failure. 
The  systematic  implication  that  the  level  of  aspiration  can  and 
must  be  regarded  as  an  instance  of  a  frame  of  reference  within 
which  ego  levels  are  set  was  not  made  in  Hoppe's  original  formu- 
lation, but  further  evidence  for  such  an  interpretation  has  accumu- 
lated with  subsequent  work. 

Frank  has  demonstrated  that  when  the  level  of  aspiration  is  set 
within  a  frame  of  reference  clearly  involving  the  ego  it  is  likely 
to  differ  from  non-ego-involving  levels  of  aspiration  by  being  con- 
sistently higher  than  the  level  of  past  performance. 

Changes  in  the  level  of  performance  in  one  task  affect  the  first  level 
of  aspiration  in  another  even  when  the  level  of  aspiration  is  solely  an 
objective  estimate,  but  can  affect  the  remaining  levels  of  aspiration 
only  when  the  ego-level  is  involved  in  both  tasks.  [19,  170] 

Frank  suggests  that 

The  size  of  the  difference  between  the  average  level  of  aspiration 
and  the  median  level  of  past  performance  is  due  to  the  involvement  of 
the  ego-level  of  the  individual  in  the  task,  as  shown  by  self-competi- 
tion or  social  pressure.  [20,  293] 

He  notes,  too,  in  his  1941  summary,  that,  for  some  people, 

The  level  of  aspiration  may  be  used  to  help  protect  the  ego  from 
the  effects  of  failure  by  being  kept  resolutely  high  despite  poor  per- 
formance. [21,  224] 

He  further  states: 

The  level  of  aspiration  situation  is  usually  a  threat  to  the  subject's 
self-esteem  in  that  he  must  not  only  exhibit  his  ability  for  some  one 


EGO-INVOLVEMENTS  IN  LABORATORY  TASKS  121 

else,  but  must  openly  commit  himself  as  to  his  expectation  of  future 
achievement.  .  .  .  Involvement  of  the  subject's  self-esteem  may  often 
be  inferred  from  tension,  obvious  effort  to  do  well,  acute  awareness  of 
the  experimenter,  and  other  signs  that  he  regards  his  own  "worth"  as 
involved.  [21,  223] 

A  notable  contribution  to  the  conceptualization  of  the  meaning 
of  the  term  "level  of  aspiration"  was  made  by  Chapman  and  Volk- 
mann  when  they  pointed  out,  as  we  mentioned  earlier,  that 

The  conditions  which  govern  the  setting  of  a  level  of  aspiration 
(Anspruchsniveau) ,  in  the  sense  of  an  estimate  of  one's  future  per- 
formance in  a  given  task,  may  be  regarded  as  a  special  case  of  the 
effect  upon  a  judgment  of  the  frame  of  reference  within  which  it  is 
executed.  [13,225] 

In  chapter  4  we  described  their  experiment  in  another  connec- 
tion. It  may  be  recalled  that  some  of  their  experimental  groups 
were  given  in  advance  of  the  task  (recognition  of  various  authors 
of  literary  passages)  the  score  supposedly  made  by  other  groups 
which  varied  in  their  prestige  such  as  literary  critics  and  WPA 
workers.  The  result  here  was  that  those  who  compared  them- 
selves to  a  superior  group  lowered  their  aspiration  level,  while 
those  who  compared  themselves  to  an  inferior  group  raised  their 
aspiration  level.  In  the  second  experiment  the  subjects  were 
allowed  two  performances  on  a  mental  test  before  any  attempt 
was  made  to  change  their  level  of  aspiration  by  comparison  of  their 
results  with  those  of  various  other  groups.  Under  these  condi- 
tions, there  was  no  significant  change  in  the  level  of  aspiration. 

The  subject's  own  previous  scores  provided  the  most  effective  an- 
choring. [13,  235] 

In  addition  to  the  information  these  experiments  provide  con- 
cerning the  relative  acceptance  of  imposed  norms  when  the  stimu- 
lus situation  is  variously  structured,  they  demonstrate  that  whether 
a  level  of  aspiration  is  socially  imposed  as  it  was  in  their  first 
experiment,  or  whether  it  is  based  on  some  definite  anchoring 
from  past  experience,  it  must  be  regarded  as  an  ego-involving 
frame  of  reference.  In  the  first  experiment,  a  subject  compares 
himself  with  others  of  a  superior  or  inferior  position,  using  his 


122  EXPERIMENTS  ON  EGO-INVOLVEMENTS 

own  status  as  an  anchoring  point.  In  the  second  experiment, 
where  judgments  of  future  performance  were  based  on  past  ex- 
perience, the  individual  identifies  himself  with  his  own  past  per- 
formance and,  as  the  authors  point  out: 

What  the  subject  has  himself  accomplished  with  labor  is  likely  to 
have  "ego  value";  it  means  more  to  him  than  does  the  verbally  re- 
ported accomplishment  of  someone  else.  The  subject  accepts  his  own 
work  with  satisfaction  if  it  seems  to  be  of  high  grade;  he  may  still 
accept  it,  under  the  protection  of  some  rationalization,  if  it  seems  to  be 
of  low  grade.  In  the  second  place,  the  subject's  previous  scores  pro- 
vide the  most  objective  basis  for  predicting  his  future  ones,  and  the 
subject  will  use  this  basis  if  he  values  objectivity.  [13,  235] 

Since  the  subjects  were  college  graduates  and  students  in  extension 
courses  in  psychology,  the  authors'  assumption  that  objective  esti- 
mates will  have  some  ego  value  seems  entirely  plausible. 

In  1940  Sears  went  farther  than  anyone  had  to  date  in  making 
explicit  what  lay  beyond  the  "level  of  aspiration"  and  why  it  in- 
volved the  ego.  After  noting  Frank's  observations  that  ego-in- 
volvement may  be  either  a  matter  of  "self-competition"  where  the 
individual  attempts  to  maintain  his  status  or  due  to  "an  aware- 
ness of  social  pressure,"  and  after  calling  attention  to  Gould's  in- 
vestigations [23]  which  pointed  out  the  importance  of  knowing 
an  individual's  attitudes  and  the  different  meanings  tasks  might 
have  for  different  individuals  or  for  the  same  individual  in  dif- 
ferent situations,  Sears  formulates  her  problem  in  such  a  way  as 
to  bring  these  variables  together  systematically  and  to  relate  them 
to  social  norms  which  can  and  do  become  interiorized  as  ego- 
involving  frames  of  reference. 

Such  differences  in  the  individual's  perception  of  the  task  in  relation 
to  himself  may  be  considered  to  have  been  built  up  in  somewhat  the 
following  fashion:  the  child  is  informed,  by  example  and  precept  of 
prestigeful  persons,  as  to  what  are  the  valued  activities  in  the  particular 
culture  or  subculture  of  which  he  is  a  member.  These  values,  incor- 
porated in  the  ego,  become  reference  points  for  self-judgments  of 
success  or  failure;  that  is,  the  child  cannot  succeed  or  fail  in  an  activ- 
ity which  has  for  him  no  ego  value.  In  those  activities  in  which  he 
has  ego-involvement,  he  can,  and  does,  succeed  and  fail  [57,  499] 


EGO-INVOLVEMENTS  IN  LABORATORY  TASKS  123 

Scars  set  out  to  test  the  "reasonable  assumption"  that  "differences 
in  success  will  influence  the  individual's  anticipation  of  future 
gratification  in  the  further  performance"  of  tasks  possessing  ego 
value. 

She  chose  as  her  subjects  children  of  fourth,  fifth,  and  sixth 
grade  standing  where  "cultural  pressures  toward  achievement  are 
exerted  on  the  child  from  all  sides  in  this  field."  [500]  Or,  as 
Sears  puts  it,  a  child  in  the  age  range  of  9  to  12  has  "had  sufficient 
experience  in  school  to  form  definite  attitudes  with  respect  to  his 
excellence  of  performance  in  the  academic  work."  Her  experi- 
mental group  consisted  of  a  "success"  group  of  children  who  had 
had  uniformly  high  academic  standing,  a  "failure"  group,  and  a 
"differential"  group  composed  of  children  who  had  been  suc- 
cessful in  reading  and  unsuccessful  in  arithmetic.  Reading  and 
arithmetic  tasks  were  assigned  and  were  given  as  speed  tests. 
There  were  three  experimental  conditions:  the  first  a  rieutral  one, 
the  second  a  "success-conditioned"  where  children  were  highly 
praised  for  their  work  no  matter  what  it  was  objectively,  and  the 
third  a  "failure-conditioned"  where  children,  irrespective  of  their 
performances,  were  told  they  had  made  many  mistakes  and  gone 
very  slowly  with  respect  to  other  members  of  the  group. 

She  found  that 

Self-confident,  successful  children  react  to  the  level  of  aspiration 
situation  in  a  similar  way,  whereas  unsuccessful  children,  lacking  in 
confidence,  may  adopt  one  of  a  number  of  different  behavior  tech- 
niques in  this  situation.  Furthermore,  experimentally  induced  success 
brings  the  reactions  of  all  subjects  in  regard  to  level  of  aspiration  into 
a  more  homogeneous  distribution  than  do  the  neutral  conditions  of 
stimulation.  [57,  526] 

Experimentally  induced  success  provides  social  norms  for  the  indi- 
vidual which  induce  him  to  believe  that  he  has  been  and  is  performing 
much  better  than  the  average  .  .  .  similarly,  experimentally  induced 
failure  provides  a  condition  of  insecurity  for  the  subjects  of  all  groups. 
[57,552/.] 

Sears  points  out  the  link  made  by  Chapman  and  Volkmann  be- 
tween the  level  of  aspiration  and  the  effect  of  a  frame  of  refer- 


124  EXPERIMENTS  ON  EGO-INVOLVEMENTS 

cnce  on  judgment  and  goes  on  to  show  that  the  level  of  aspiration 
is  set  with  respect  to  "the  perceived  social  norms."  [529]  She  con- 
cludes that 

The  cultural  pressure  to  excel  and  to  keep  the  performance  improv- 
ing, plus  the  cognizance  of  the  position  of  the  self  relative  to  social 
norms,  seem  to  account  for  most  of  the  results  obtained  in  the  present 
investigation.  [57,  528] 

The  earlier  studies  of  Greenberg  in  1932  [25]  and  Rosenzweig 
in  1933  [55]  furnish  experimental  evidence  that  the  level  of  aspira- 
tion with  its  involvement  of  the  ego  does  not  appear  genetically 
until  the  child  has  formed  some  conception  of  his  "self,"  has  de- 
veloped a  sense  of  "pride,"  or  of  status  which  he  feels  he  must 
maintain  within  a  group.  "Success"  and  "failure"  in  the  accom- 
plishment of  various  assigned  tasks  are  meaningless  until  the  ego 
is  sufficiently  developed  to  serve  as  a  basic  anchorage.  Anderson 
and  Brandt  [3]  studied  the  levels  of  aspiration  of  fifth  grade 
children  for  performances  when  each  child  knew  his  own  pre- 
vious score,  his  standing  in  the  group,  and  the  group  norm. 
Since  no  child  knew  the  scores  made  by  any  other  specific  child, 
direct  "social  pressure"  and  "competition"  were  minimal.  In  this 
situation,  the  explicit  norm  of  the  group  served  as  an  anchorage 
for  judgments  of  future  performances.  The  investigators  report 
that  "the  goals  or  levels  of  aspiration  of  these  children  tended  to 
converge  on  what  for  the  group  was  mediocrity."  [231]  Other 
results  were  contrary  to  Frank's  definition  of  failure  as  a  level  of 
performance  below  the  level  of  aspiration  "regardless  of  its  abso- 
lute goodness."  The  children  in  this  study  who  won  scores  in 
the  lower  quartile  tended  to  set  their  goals  "considerably  above 
their  achievement,  whereas  in  the  upper  ranks  of  performance 
the  tendency  is  just  the  reverse."  [220]  As  the  authors  concluded: 
"The  goals  of  these  children  are  related  to  the  'goodness'  of  past 
achievement."  [225]  And,  as  we  have  seen,  they  are  also  related 
to  the  past  achievement  of  the  group.  The  average  performance 
of  this  group  of  children  improved  significantly  more  during  a 
series  of  trials  than  did  those  of  a  control  group  who  knew 
neither  their  own  score  nor  their  ranking  in  the  group. 

In  their  study  of  the  effect  of  relative  standing  in  an  experimen- 


EGO-INVOLVEMENTS  IN  LABORATORY  TASKS  125 

tal  social  group  to  the  level  of  aspiration,  Hilgard,  Sait,  and 
Magaret  [28]  found  a  tendency  for  those  whose  performance  was 
above  the  group  norm  to  estimate  their  future  performance  too 
low,  whereas  those  whose  performances  were  lower  than  the  group 
norm  tended  to  overestimate  their  future  performance.  Since  the 
estimates  of  future  performances  of  one  subject  were  not  known 
to  the  other  subject,  the  experimenters  conclude  that  "group  pres- 
sure can  be  used  to  explain  the  tendency  to  estimate  towards  the 
mean  of  the  group  only  if  the  desire  for  social  conformity  is  some- 
how internalized."  [421] 

Several  studies  have  been  reported  which  show  that  unless  and 
until  there  is  some  ego-involvement  no  level  of  aspiration  is  set 
and  the  individual  has  no  concern  about  his  own  status.  Only 
when  an  individual  judges  his  own  performance  in  terms  of  a 
standard  he  wants  to  maintain  or  achieve,  does  any  generality  in 
levels  of  aspiration  appear  with  respect  to  various  tasks.  Klein 
and  Schoenfeld  [41]  found  that,  when  their  subjects  were  in  a 
neutral  non-ego-involving  atmosphere,  the  confidence  they  had  in 
their  ability  to  perform  various  tasks  depended  almost  entirely  on 
the  nature  and  difficulty  of  the  tasks  themselves,  whereas  in  defi- 
nitely ego-involving  situations  (where  subjects  were  told  their 
scores  would  be  sent  to  the  personnel  bureau  of  the  college)  a 
"generality  of  confidence"  appeared.  That  the  subjects  did  actu- 
ally feel  personally  involved  in  the  second  part  of  the  experiment 
is  indicated  by  the  report  that  in  the  second  situation  they  exerted 
greater  effort  and  experienced  considerable  anxiety.  In  this  ex- 
periment the  person  obviously  becomes  involved  because  perform- 
ance of  the  task  is  believed  to  have  relevance  to  a  value  already 
interiorized  as  part  of  the  ego,  namely,  attendance  and  gradua- 
tion from  college  and  a  respectable  standing  with  college  author- 
ities. 

Holt  [30]  found  that,  if  his  subjects  had  any  ego-involvement 
in  the  abilities  needed  to  perform  his  experimental  task,  the  esti- 
mates thus  made  of  their  performances  were  determined  signifi- 
cantly less  by  the  objective  situational  variables  than  when  there 
was  little  ego-involvement.  If  there  is  less  ego-involvement,  "a 
level  of  aspiration  is  of  a  piece  with  the  total  response  to  the  situa- 
tion of  the  experiment;  in  that  sense  it  is  more  specific,  more 


126  EXPERIMENTS  ON  EGO-INVOLVEMENTS 

peripheral  and  responsive  to  outer  environmental  forces."  [314] 
Heathers'  [26]  study  of  generality  in  the  level  of  aspiration  points 
to  the  same  conclusion.  In  a  study  of  saturation  with  tasks  after 
repeated  performance,  Karsten  discovered  in  1928  [37]  that  her 
subjects  became  more  rapidly  saturated  (annoyed,  impatient,  emo- 
tionally upset)  with  tasks  judged  to  be  very  pleasant  or  very  un- 
pleasant than  with  indifferent  tasks  or  those  only  mildly  pleasant 
or  unpleasant.  As  Koffka  concludes  from  this  experiment  "the 
relation  of  the  task  to  the  Ego  is  the  decisive  factor.  In  the  indif- 
ferent task  the  Ego  is  not  'engaged/  with  the  result  that  Ego  ten- 
sions are  less  easily  produced."  [42,  412] 

In  an  ingenious  experiment  where  pairs  of  subjects  worked  to- 
gether, one  performing  a  task  and  estimating  his  performance 
ahead  of  time,  the  other  estimating  what  his  partner  would  do, 
McGehee  [49]  found  considerably  less  variability  among  the  levels 
of  aspiration  of  those  actually  involved  in  the  task  than  in  the 
estimates  of  the  observers.  McGehee  concludes  that  this  difference 
"seems  to  lie  in  the  fact  that  the  ego-levels  of  the  subjects  are  more 
involved  in  the  erection  of  levels  of  aspiration  than  in  making  a 
judgment."  [14]  This  distinction  between  level  of  aspiration  and 
judgment  obscures  the  relationship  between  them  and  violates 
what  we  know  of  the  process  of  judgment  itself,  namely,  that  all 
judgments  take  place  within  some  frame  of  reference.  The  esti- 
mates of  McGehee's  subjects  who  were  actually  engaged  in  the 
tasks  are  obviously  just  as  much  judgments  as  the  estimates  of  the 
observers.  But,  whereas  one  set  of  judgments  is  based  upon  frames 
or  anchorages  that  are  ego-involved,  the  other  set  of  judgments 
is  based  on  non-ego-involved  frames  or  anchorages  and  is  there- 
fore more  determined  by  immediate  past  performance  of  the  par- 
ticipating partner  or  other  variables  in  the  situation  itself. 

EGO-INVOLVEMENTS  AS  A  SET  FOR  LEARNING 

In  chapter  4  we  cited  some  of  the  studies  that  show  the  close 
relationship  between  memory  and  frame  of  reference.  In  all  these 
studies  dealing  with  everyday  life  material,  it  is  clear  that  the  in- 
vestigators were  concerned  with  ego-involving  attitudes — attitudes 
that  have  been  learned,  largely  as  social  values;  that  the  individual 


EGO-INVOLVEMENTS  AS  A  SET  FOR  LEARNING  127 

identifies  himself  with,  and  makes  a  part  of  himself;  and  that  have 
affective  properties  of  varying  degrees  of  intensity.  Thus,  the 
effect  Seeleman  [58]  found  of  the  attitude  toward  the  Negro  on 
the  memory  of  pictures  of  whites  and  Negroes  clearly  provokes 
a  value  judgment  with  which  most  Americans  at  the  present  time 
identify  themselves  in  one  way  or  another  and  in  different  degrees 
of  intensity.  Zillig's  well  known  study  [66]  of  the  differential 
ability  of  men  and  women  to  recall  items  favorable  to  their  own 
sex  implicitly  involves  or  reflects  sex  differences  in  ego  composi- 
tion as  learned.  Edwards  [17,  18]  showed  the  superior  recogni- 
tion of  items  favorable  to  a  political  point  of  view  which  the 
individual  espouses  and  the  extent  to  which  an  individual  ration- 
alizes answers  to  factual  statements  not  in  harmony  with  his  atti- 
tude, implicitly  demonstrating  what  is  common  knowledge, 
namely,  that  in  the  contemporary  United  States,  an  individual 
regards  the  party  he  votes  for  as  his  party,  party  platforms  as  his 
expressed  point  of  view,  and  party  candidates  as  his  candidates. 
Similarly,  the  results  of  Levine  and  Murphy  [43]  showed  that 
both  the  learning  and  forgetting  of  material  favorable  to  or  against 
the  Soviet  Union  is  affected  by  one's  attitude  toward  Communism, 
is  most  definitely  concerned  with  ego-involved  attitudes,  as  any- 
one can  testify  who  observes  the  identification  an  individual  Com- 
munist makes  with  Marxism  or  the  Communist  Party,  or  who 
has  seen  the  emotional  involvement  of  those  who  oppose  Com- 
munism as  something  which  threatens  "all  that  they  hold  dear." 
Birch  [6]  also  noted  the  definite  tendency  for  his  subjects  who 
approved  a  statement  labeled  "communist"  to  write  more  com- 
ments than  usual,  dissociating  themselves  from  Communism  as 
such  or  otherwise  qualifying  their  answers. 

Wallen's  experiments  in  1941  [63,  64]  yielded  clear-cut  evidence 
that  the  selective  forgetting  of  ego-involved  material  differs  sig- 
nificantly from  the  selective  forgetting  of  material  unrelated  to 
the  ego.  Subjects  were  given  a  list  of  adjectives  on  which  they 
made  self -evaluations.  They  were  then  divided  into  two  groups: 
one  group  was  later  given  orally  a  list  of  bogus  ratings  on  the 
same  adjectives  supposedly  made  by  some  acquaintance  on  the 
subject;  the  other  group  was  later  given  bogus  ratings  but  were 
told  these  were  ratings  on  unidentified  people.  His  results  showed 


128  EXPERIMENTS  ON  EGO-INVOLVEMENTS 

that  the  recall  of  the  former  group  was  significantly  higher  than 
the  latter.  In  the  former  group  bogus  ratings  which  agreed  with 
the  subject's  own  evaluation  were  considerably  better  remembered 
than  others,  whereas  in  the  second  group  no  such  tendency  oc- 
curred: those  who  had  bogus  ratings  on  themselves  recalled  de- 
sirable ratings  more  accurately  than  those  they  regarded  as  un- 
desirable, so  much  so  that  the  final  recall  was  actually  much  more 
like  the  original  list  of  self-evaluations  than  the  material  actually 
presented  for  recall.  Wallen  believed  that  "the  difference  in  in- 
structions for  the  two  groups  can  be  cogently  interpreted  only  in 
ego  terms"  [64,  36]  and  that  "some  assumption  regarding  an  ego 
is  needed  in  interpreting  our  results."  [36]  A  later  experiment 
by  Shaw  and  Spooner  [59],  again  involving  a  list  of  adjectives  on 
which  a  specified  person  was  to  be  rated,  bogus  ratings,  and  de- 
layed recall,  found  superior  recall  of  the  adjectives  in  line  with  the 
subject's  own  initial  characterization  or  judgments. 

These  results  are  comparable  in  their  implications  to  the  con- 
clusions reached  by  Wolff  [65]  and  Huntley  [33]  in  their  studies 
of  judgments  of  expressive  behavior  (voice  recording,  handwrit- 
ing, profiles,  and  the  like).  They  found  their  subjects  generally 
made  much  more  favorable  judgments  of  samples  of  their  own 
expressive  behavior  and  that  in  nearly  all  cases  judgments  of  one's 
own  behavior  or  features  took  on  the  characteristics  of  an  extreme 
reaction,  even  though  few  of  the  subjects  consciously  recognized 
which  behavior  or  feature  presented  was  their  own.  Such  experi- 
ments demonstrate  in  a  significant  way  that  an  individual's  body 
and  his  physical  personal  characteristics  constitute  an  integral  part 
of  his  ego. 

In  a  study  of  repression,  Rosenzweig  [56]  set  up  his  experiment 
so  that  one  group  would  be  "aroused  in  a  personal  way"  whereas 
the  other  group  was  in  a  relatively  neutral  situation.  Both  groups 
were  asked  to  recall  tasks  they  had  been  assigned,  only  half  of 
which  they  had  been  allowed  to  complete.  Differences  in  recall 
were  clear-cut:  those  who  had  become  personally  involved  remem- 
bering more  finished  tasks,  those  not  involved  more  unfinished. 
Similar  findings  were  later  reported  by  Lewis  and  Franklin.  [45] 
Alper's  results  [2]  show  very  neatly  that  retention  and  recall  are 
considerably  different  for  subjects  who  are  or  are  not  ego-involved 


EGO-INVOLVEMENTS  AS  A  SET  FOR  LEARNING  129 

(she  uses  the  terms  "ego-oriented"  and  "task-oriented")  and  that 
the  three  "classical"  laws  of  learning  she  analyzed  can  be  said  to 
hold  only  when  there  is  no  ego-involvement.2 

In  his  study  of  recognition,  published  in  1911,  Claparede  pointed 
out  that  "One  must  distinguish  between  two  sorts  of  connections: 
those  which  establish  themselves  eventually  between  ideas,  and 
those  which  establish  themselves  between  the  ideas  and  that  which 
constitutes  the  Ego,  the  personality.  In  the  case  of  purely  passive 
or  reflex  association  of  ideas  the  first  kind  of  connections  will 
function  alone;  in  the  case  of  voluntary  recall  or  of  recognition, 
where  the  Ego  is  involved,  the  second  kind  would  play  its  part." 8 
[14,  56]  Koffka  relates  how  he  refused  to  take  Claparede's  for- 
mulations seriously  in  his  own  early  thinking  because  the  problem 
of  the  Ego  was  not  prominent  in  psychology.  After  praising 
Claparede's  insight  and  reviewing  Claparede's  theory  as  it  related 
to  his  own  conception  of  trace  systems  and  the  Ego,  Koffka  con- 
cludes that 

...  in  the  structure  of  the  behavioural  environment  there  are  things 
close  to  and  remote  from  the  Ego  and  even  some  that  have  practically 
no  Ego-connection.  According  to  the  theory,  and  to  all  appearances  in 
conformity  with  the  facts,  the  former  are  better  recognized  than  the 
latter.  In  many  cases  the  Ego-object  relationship  will  be,  at  least 
partly,  due  to  the  interests  and  attitudes  of  the  Ego.  Thus  whatever 
has  interested  us,  attracted  our  attention,  is  relatively  easily  recognized. 
[42,595/J 

And  Koffka  goes  on  to  quote  Maccurdy's  contention  that  "The 
things  we  deny  ever  having  done,  in  the  face  of  ample  testimony 

2  Alper  concludes  that  ego-involved  performances  go  beyond  Ebbinghaus*  three 
laws,  whereas  Bruner  states  that  his  results  go  beyond  Weber's  law  (p.  75). 
Variations  in  other  psychological  functions  (such  as  "social  perception")  are  being 
obtained  which  go  beyond  classical  laws  in  those  fields.  All  these  "going  be- 
yonds"  can  be  accounted  for  in  a  conceptual  scheme  if  they  are  taken  as  special 
cases  of  referential  frameworks  with  different  factors  (anchorages)  coming  into 
play  to  modify  experiences  one  way  or  another.  Likewise,  the  apparently  contra- 
dictory variations  sometimes  obtained  in  level  of  aspiration  experiments  done 
under  different  conditions  can  be  easily  explained  if  they  are  seen  as  so  many 
special  cases  of  referential  frames  and  different  anchorages  coming  into  play  to 
produce  the  results.  This  point  has  been  nicely  formulated  by  Chapman  and 
Volkmann  (p.  121)  and  Pauline  Sears  (p.  122). 

8  This  translated  Quotation  is  taken  from  Koffka.  [42,  5951 


130  EXPERIMENTS  ON  EGO-INVOLVEMENTS 

from  honest  observers,  are  acts  performed  'absent-mindedly,'  auto- 
matically. Automatic  behavior  has  no  me-ness  attached  to  it."  [46, 
124] 

Those  who  try  to  account  for  learning  on  the  basis  of  some 
relatively  static  conception  may  object  that  the  differential  learn- 
ing found  in  ego-  versus  non-ego-involved  situations  represents 
nothing  more  than  the  operation  of  different  conditions  of  learn- 
ing, and  that  the  introduction  of  "ego-involved"  situations  is  there- 
fore quite  unnecessary.  We  have  emphasized  that  we  regard  the 
ego  as  a  product  of  learning.  And  when  the  psychology  of  learn- 
ing is  fully  understood,  we  shall  have  much  more  precise  knowl- 
edge of  the  ego's  development  than  we  do  now.  What  the  ex- 
periments on  ego-involvement  and  memory  show  is  the  necessity 
of  recognizing  the  fact  that  the  learning  situation  is  differently 
organized  for  the  individual  when  he  feels  that  he  is  somehow 
involved  and  that  this  organization,  this  interest  determination  or 
interest  setting  as  Bartlett  calls  it,  affects  what  will  be  learned  and 
how  learning  will  take  place. 

EGO-INVOLVEMENTS  SHOWN  IN  THE  INTENSITY  OF  ATTITUDES 

In  previous  chapters  we  established  the  fact  that  attitudes  are 
learned  either  as  the  result  of  direct  contact  with  a  person  or  a 
group  or  by  means  of  transmission  of  social  norms  through  short- 
cut verbal  value  judgments.  We  also  pointed  out  that  most  atti- 
tudes of  everyday  life  concerned  with  concrete  social  situations 
have  affective  and  motivational  properties  of  varying  degrees. 
And  we  cited  numerous  studies  which  showed  the  effect  of  these 
attitudes  on  perception,  judgment,  and  behavior.  We  pointed  out 
that  the  affective  properties  of  attitudes  derived  from  noninstinc- 
tual  sources  is  due  to  the  fact  that  these  attitudes  were  formed  in 
relation  to  social  values  or  norms  which  in  themselves  are  stand- 
ardized affective  fixations,  (p.  20/.) 

We  were  careful  throughout  our  earlier  discussion  of  the  prop- 
erties of  attitudes  to  state  that  affect  or  emotional  involvement  was 
of  varying  degrees.  It  is  obvious  that  a  sophisticated  Negro  who 
has  suffered  job  discrimination  will  follow  the  course  of  fair  em- 
ployment legislation  much  more  anxiously  than  a  white  person 


EGO-INVOLVEMENTS  SHOWN  IN  THE  INTENSITY  OF  ATTITUDES      131 

or  another  Negro  who  has  only  a  mild  hope  that  something  can 
be, done  to  improve  the  status  of  Negroes.  We  know  that,  by  and 
large,  a  big  employer  or  major  stockholder  is  more  likely  to  take 
some  overt  action  to  counter  union  demands  for  higher  wages 
than  some  relatively  disinterested  person  whose  attitude  toward 
the  union  may,  however,  be  just  as  unfavorable  when  judged  from 
conversation  or  measured  by  an  ordinary  attitude  scale.  We  have 
seen  since  the  end  of  World  War  II  how  the  Jews  in  the  United 
States  have  taken  the  initiative  in  forming  committees  or  groups 
of  one  kind  or  another  to  seek  some  kind  of  government  or  inter- 
governmental help  for  the  tortured  and  displaced  Jews  of  Europe, 
while  many  other  Americans  whose  attitudes  toward  the  European 
Jews  may  have  been  just  as  favorable  as  that  of  some  of  their  Jew- 
ish countrymen  did  nothing  more  than  read  with  a  sigh  accounts 
about  these  efforts  of  assistance.  In  such  cases  we  say  that  "self- 
interest"  is  involved.  And  we  mean  by  this  that  individuals  iden- 
tify themselves  with  different  degrees  of  intensity  to  various  social 
causes,  principles,  ideals.  In  other  words,  the  intensity  with  which 
we  hold  those  attitudes  formed  in  relation  to  social  values  or  those 
personal  attitudes  formed  in  relation  to  particular  surroundings 
or  experiences  depends  on  the  degree  to  which  those  attitudes  are 
ego-involved.  Bruner's  finding  that  poor  children  overestimated 
the  size  of  coins  more  than  rich  children  (p.  75)  points  to  the 
fact  that  the  value  of  coins  as  perceptual  objects  was  clearly  de- 
termined by  ego-involving  attitudes  which  varied  in  their  intensity 
for  the  two  groups  of  children. 

Studies  concerned  with  the  intensity  of  attitude  and  the  relation 
of  intensity  to  the  content  or  direction  of  attitude  can,  it  seems 
to  us,  only  be  interpreted  by  regarding  the  reference  scale  by  means 
of  which  a  person  judges  the  relative  importance  to  him  of  the 
attitudes  he  holds  as  ego-involved.  This  degree  of  ego-involve- 
ment, this  intensity  of  attitudes,  will  determine  in  large  part  which 
attitudes  he  will  cling  to,  how  annoyed  or  frustrated  he  will  feel 
when  his  attitudes  are  opposed,  what  action  (within  the  range  of 
his  individual  temperament  and  ability)  he  will  take  to  further 
his  point  of  view. 

In  an  attempt  to  draw  a  distinction  between  the  intensity  of  an 
attitude  and  its  direction  and  to  determine  the  relationship  of 


132  EXPERIMENTS  ON  EGO-INVOLVEMENTS 

these  two  variables,  Cantril  [11]  constructed  two  simple  attitude 
scales,  involving  several  alternatives  which  were  rated  by  judges 
as  to  the  place  each  alternative  statement  should  occupy  on  an 
attitude  continuum  which  ranged  from  most  favorable  to  most 
unfavorable  attitude.  Two  scales,  one  dealing  with  the  attitude 
toward  the  Negro  and  the  other  with  government  regulation  of 
business,  were  included  in  separate  nationwide  samples  of  1200 
adult  white  people  in  the  United  States.  After  an  individual  had 
indicated  the  statement  that  best  expressed  his  own  point  of  view, 
he  was  asked  how  strongly  he  held  this  opinion— very  strongly, 
fairly  strongly,  or  not  caring  much  one  way  or  the  other. 

The  results,  shown  in  Charts  1  and  2,  indicate  that,  the  more 
extreme  an  attitude  is  in  its  direction,  the  more  intensely  it  is 
likely  to  be  held. 

Comparable  results  have  been  reported  by  Stouffer  and  his  asso- 
ciates in  their  work  with  the  Information  and  Education  Division 
of  the  Army  Service  Forces  [61].  These  investigators  used  a  bat- 
tery of  different  questions  to  construct  a  scale  on  the  "content" 
of  an  attitude  and  then  measured  the  intensity  (or  certainty)  of 
opinion.  These  scales  were  administered  to  Army  populations. 
Their  results,  too,  show  that  intensity  is  highly  correlated  with  the 
extremity  of  an  attitude.  "A  series  of  questions  which  form  both 
a  content  and  an  intensity  scale,  will  produce  an  invariant  U-shaped 
curve."  [4]  This  U-shaped  curve,  seen  in  the  charts,  may  tend  to 
err  on  the  conservative  side,  in  view  of  the  fact  that  the  results 
were  obtained  by  the  usual  interviewing  method  where  the  inter- 
viewer asks  the  respondent  his  opinion.  From  comparisons  of 
opinions  obtained  with  comparable  samples  when  "secret,"  as  op- 
posed to  "nonsecret,"  ballots  are  used,  we  know  that  there  is  a 
tendency  for  people  to  answer  in  the  direction  of  what  they  re- 
gard as  the  socially  approved  norm  when  asked  their  opinion  on 
nonsecret  ballots  dealing  with  controversial  issues.  This  tendency 
to  conformity  on  the  nonsecret  ballot  is  itself  obviously  a  further 
demonstration  of  the  fact  that  individuals  feel  themselves  per- 
sonally involved  in  expressions  of  their  opinions  and  seek  to  raise 
their  status  in  the  eyes  of  the  interviewer.  This  tendency  was 
significantly  revealed,  for  example,  in  the  finding  that  13  per  cent 


EGO-INVOLVEMENTS  SHOWN  IN  THE  INTENSITY  OF  ATnTUDES     133 


80 


70 


60 


50 


\J6* 


21%, 


40 


0123456789        10 
Unfavorable  Favorable 

CHART  1.    Direction  and  intensity  of  opinion  toward  the  Negro. 


80 


70 


60 


50 


40 


9% 


,23% 


0123456789       10 
Unfavorable  Favorable 

CHART  2.  Direction  and  intensity  of  opinion  toward  Government  ownership. 


134  EXPERIMENTS  ON  EQO-INVOLVEMENTS 

more  of  the  secret  ballot  group  than  of  the  interviewed  group 
stated  that  they  Jiad  no  schooling.  [62] 

It  is  the  intensity  with  which  an  attitude  is  held,  the  degree  of 
ego-involvement  it  has  for  the  individual,  that  in  large  part  de- 
termines the  consistency  of  opinion,  the  tendency  to  discern  the 
relationship  of  a  variety  of  specific  issues  to  some  basic  frame  of 
reference.  Katz'  analysis  [39]  of  public  opinion  poll  results  ob- 
tained before  Pearl  Harbor  shows  that  those  Americans  who 
strongly  believed  we  should  help  England  even  at  the  risk  of 
getting  into  the  war  ourselves  were  more  consistent  in  other  re- 
lated attitudes  than  those  who  shared  this  opinion  but  did  not 
hold  it  so  intensely.  A  scale  constructed  to  measure  the  intensity 
of  political  interest  rather  sharply  distinguished  voters  from  non- 
voters  and  aided  in  the  prediction  of  the  political  behavior  of 
various  groups.  [10, 191  f.]  Postman  and  Zimmerman  [53]  have 
shown  that,  the  more  intensely  an  attitude  is  held,  the  shorter  is 
the  decision  time  of  judgments  based  on  that  attitude. 

STATUS  AND  CLASS  IDENTIFICATION  AS  EGO-INVOLVEMENTS 

We  have  stated  that  one  important  reason  why  attitudes  are 
affectively  charged  is  the  fact  that  many  attitudes  prescribe  the 
individual's  relationship,  status,  or  role  with  respect  to  other  in- 
dividuals or  groups  (such  as  teacher,  worker,  boss,  minister,  assist- 
ant). And  experiences  connected  with  status  are  affectively 
charged  (p.  21).  We  pointed  out  further  that  attitudes  related 
to  role  or  status  are  ego-involved. 

In  his  discussion  of  the  "social"  self,  James  made  it  quite  clear 
that  status  identification  often  determines  behavior  in  everyday 
life.  It  is  a  man's 

.  .  ,  image  in  the  eyes  of  his  own  "set,"  which  exalts  or  condemns 
him  as  he  conforms  or  not  to  certain  requirements  that  may  not  be 
made  of  one  in  another  walk  of  life.  Thus  a  layman  may  abandon  a 
city  infected  with  cholera;  but  a  priest  or  doctor  would  think  such  an 
act  incompatible  with  his  honor.  A  soldier's  honor  requires  him  to 
fight  or  to  die  under  circumstances  where  another  man  can  apologize 


STATUS  AND  CLASS  IDENTIFICATION  AS  EGO-INVOLVEMENTS       135 

or  run  away  with  no  stain  upon  his  social  self.  .  .  .  The  thief  must 
not  steal  from  other  thieves;  the  gambler  must  pay  his  gambling  debts, 
though  he  pay  no  other  debts  in  the  world.  [35,  294  /.] 

In  later  chapters  we  will  see  how  an  individual  identifies  him- 
self with  and  regards  as  a  part  of  himself  the  particular  constella- 
tion of  values  he  learns  from  his  environment.  On  the  basis  of 
this  learning  the  individual  defines  his  own  role  or  status:  he 
learns  what  group  or  groups  he  belongs  to;  what  other  groups 
are  regarded  as  "higher"  or  "lower"  than  his  own;  what  groups 
are  to  be  regarded  as  enemies,  antagonists,  or  competitors;  what 
as  allies,  helpers,  or  friends.  For  example,  the  behavior  of  the 
American  GJ.  abroad  during  and  after  World  War  II  often  em- 
phasized in  striking  fashion  to  people  in  foreign  countries  the 
status  roles  defined  by  their  own  norms.  Hilgard  has  reported 
some  of  his  observations  made  during  the  spring  of  1946,  of  the 
effect  of  G.I.  behavior  on  the  Japanese: 

Japanese  etiquette  requires  courtesy  to  those  above  you  in  status, 
and  does  not  demand  much  consideration  between  equals.  .  .  .  The 
lack  of  regard  for  status,  foreign  to  the  Japanese,  is  admired.  GJ.'s 
like  to  ride  in  rickshaws,  for  the  novelty  of  the  experience,  but  they 
are  just  as  likely  to  have  a  little  fun  pulling  one.  This  sort  of  be- 
havior would  be  unheard  of  among  Japanese,  but  it  is  not  found 
repugnant.  [29,  347] 

The  same  American  GJ.'s  would,  of  course,  probably  find  their 
ideas  of  status  violated  by  any  behavior  on  their  own  home  ground 
that  deviated  from  discrimination  concerning  race,  class,  or  sex 
prescribed  by  norms  of  their  own. 

We  have  already  mentioned  a  number  of  experiments  concerned 
with  the  effect  an  individual's  place  in  a  group  has  on  his  level 
of  aspiration.  A  considerable  body  of  data  has  by  now  accumu- 
lated from  other  controlled  studies  which  further  emphasize  the 
fact  that  a  person  defines  his  role,  his  status,  his  class,  on  the  basis 
of  the  anchorage  provided  him  by  values  which  he  has  made  part 
of  himself.  By  means  of  these  anchorages  he  makes  relevant 
judgments  both  of  his  own  place  in  society  and  the  place  of 
others. 


136  EXPERIMENTS  ON  EGO-INVOLVEMENTS 

An  especially  revealing  demonstration  of  such  an  anchorage  is 
the  study  of  Marks  [48]  on  Negro  judgments  of  skin  color  and 
personal  attractiveness.  Following  up  by  controlled  experimenta- 
tion Johnson's  field  observations  [36]  that  the  judgments  of  Negro 
youth  on  the  skin  colors  of  other  Negroes  tended  to  depend  upon 
the  rater's  own  skin  color  and  his  observation  that  more  favorable 
characteristics  tended  to  be  associated  with  lighter  colors,  Marks 
had  each  student  in  his  class  (all  Negroes)  rate  every  other  student 
on  certain  characteristics  including  skin  color,  personal  charm, 
desire  to  know  the  person  well,  on  an  eight-point  scale.  He  found 
a  high  correlation  between  ratings  on  lightness  of  skin  color  and 
ratings  on  personal  charm  and  "a  tendency  to  displace  the  ratings 
of  subjects  considered  attractive  in  the  direction  of  the  preferred 
color."  [48,  376]  Particularly  important  for  our  purposes  here  is 
the  finding  that  the  reference  scale  by  means  of  which  each  sub- 
ject judged  skin  color  was  relatively 

.  ,  .  independent  of  the  subjects  rated  but  not  of  the  rater's  own 
past  experience.  It  appears  further  that  each  judge's  rating  scale  tends 
to  be  egocentric,  Le.t  a  subject  is  seen  as  darker  or  lighter  than  the  rater 
and  judgments  are  made  accordingly.  In  such  a  scale  the  relative 
position  of  each  subject  will  be  the  same  for  the  different  raters  but 
the  absolute  position  of  a  given  subject  will  vary  from  rater  to  rater. 
[48,  374,  italics  ours] 

Marks  further  points  out  that 

The  relation  between  the  judge's  own  skin  color  and  his  rating  of 
others  seems  to  have  particular  importance  for  the  theory  of  social 
perception.  The  "egocentricity"  of  the  reference  scale  of  skin  color 
judgments  may  well  apply  to  judgments  of  any  characteristic  to  which 
social  value  is  attached,  [375] 

The  goal  of  neutral  emotional  content  frequently  involves  a  re- 
structuring of  our  social  perceptual  field.  The  individual  minimizes 
his  own  deviation  (slight  or  great)  from  the  "normal"  by  displacing 
his  perception  of  other  individuals  so  that  they  are  seen  as  above  or 
below  average  in  terms  of  their  difference  from  himself,  [48,  375] 

The  subjects  in  these  groups,  Marks  believed,  were  in  large  part 
striving  to  conform  to  an  average  or  group  norm  in  an  attempt 
to  regard  themselves  as  inconspicuous,  to  achieve  a  "neutral  emo- 


STATUS  AND  CLASS  IDENTIFICATION  AS  EGO-INVOLVEMENTS       137 

tional  content"  as  discovered  from  their  own  past  experience  in 
the  process  of  socialization. 

This  investigation  confirms  in  a  particularly  clear-cut  way  two 
points  we  should  again  emphasize:  (1)  in  the  process  of  genetic 
development  the  individual's  physical  body  and  its  characteristics 
constitute  an  early  and  important  part  of  the  ego.  Judgments  of 
the  physical  characteristics  of  others  (such  as  their  height,  their 
stoutness,  their  strength,  their  beauty)  become,  therefore,  ego- 
involving  judgments  in  which  an  individual  uses  himself  or  his 
own  characteristics  as  a  central  point  of  reference  (ch.  7).  (2)  If 
the  stimulus  field  is  relatively  well-structured,  has  certain  discern- 
ible and  compelling  objective  properties,  an  individual's  frame  or 
scale  is  almost  bound  to  be  determined  in  part  by  them.  Thus,  as 
Marks  indicates: 

The  very  dark  individual  cannot  conceive  himself  as  "neutral"  in 
color  because  his  social  environment  insists  upon  the  "objective"  facts. 
A  compromise  results,  in  which  the  deviation  may  be  minimized 
(perceived  as  less  extreme  by  the  individual  than  it  is  by  others)  but  is 
not  ignored.  [48,  375  f.] 

A  significant  study  on  the  psychology  of  status  is  that  of  Hyman. 
[34]  After  learning  from  an  intensive  interview  of  each  of  his 
subjects  something  of  the  meaning,  genesis,  criteria  of,  and  satis- 
faction with  status,  he  constructed  scales  to  measure  subjective 
status  along  several  dimensions:  general  status,  economic,  intel- 
lectual, cultural,  social,  and  physical  attractiveness.  Subjects  were 
also  asked  to  indicate  their  subjective  status  with  reference  to 
different  groups:  the  total  adult  population  in  the  United  States, 
friends  and  acquaintances,  and  their  occupational  group.  He 
found  that  "within  each  status  dimension  an  individual's  judg- 
ment of  his  status  shifts  when  reference  groups  are  changed."  [49] 
Among  other  results  reported,  the  following  are  particularly  sig- 
nificant for  our  purposes  here:  individuals  strive  for  status  with 
respect  to  those  accomplishments  or  characteristics  which  they 
most  highly  value;  when  an  individual's  status  is  approximately 
similar  to  die  status  of  the  group  he  is  using  as  a  basis  for  com- 
parison, then  he  shows  no  particular  concern  for  his  own  status, 
no  great  drive  to  achieve  a  higher  status;  persons  who  regard  the 


138  EXPERIMENTS  ON  EGO-INVOLVEMENTS 

difference  between  their  own  status  and  a  reference  group  as  being 
determined  by  a  social  system  they  disapprove  of  also  show  little 
dissatisfaction  with  their  own  status. 

Hyman  emphasizes  the  contrast  between  subjective  and  objec- 
tive status: 

The  variables  of  status  are  mediated  through  an  individual  who 
acts  selectively  in  his  choice  of  reference  group,  who  strives  selec- 
tively for  status,  whose  personal  values  affect  the  composition  of 
status  and  the  emotional  concomitants  of  a  given  status,  whose  con- 
ceptualization of  a  reference  group  may  be  different  from  its  actual 
character,  who  is  not  affected  by  all  aspects  of  the  culture  nor  by  all 
references  in  the  environment.  This  essentially  personal  aspect  of 
status  cannot  be  ignored.  We  cannot  deal  with  these  variables  inde- 
pendent of  their  meaning  to  individuals.  [34,  80] 

This  study  clearly  indicates  that  an  individual's  conception  of  his 
own  status  or  the  status  of  his  group  invariably  depends  upon  the 
other  groups  to  which  he  compares  himself  or  his  own  group.  In 
other  words,  he  uses  himself  or  his  group  as  the  anchoring  point, 
and  his  own  subjective  status  varies  according  to  the  scale  pro- 
vided. 

In  chapter  4  we  learned  from  several  independent  investigations 
[4,  40,  52]  that  individuals  easily  characterize  various  occupational 
groups  and  rank  them  according  to  a  hierarchy  based  on  the  ac- 
cepted social  norms  established  in  our  social  organization.  We 
saw  from  Davis'  study  [16]  how  children  in  Soviet  Russia  ranked 
occupations  in  quite  a  different  way  from  the  manner  they  are 
usually  ranked  in  the  United  States:  lawyers  and  bankers  were 
rated  low  in  Russia  and  skilled  workers  high. 

Gould  and  Lewis  [24]  found  that  an  individual  defines  success 
differently  when  comparing  himself  to  the  supposed  performance 
of  college  professors  than  when  comparing  himself  with  the  sup- 
posed performance  of  WPA  workers.  Similar  results  were  ob- 
tained by  Hertzman  and  Festinger  [27].  The  experiment  of 
Preston  and  Bayton  [54]  revealed  that,  although  there  was  no 
difference  in  the  level  of  aspiration  of  Negro  college  men  when 
the  experimental  group  was  told  it  was  competing  with  white 
men  and  the  control  group  told  it  was  competing  with  Negroes 


STATUS  AND  CLASS  IDENTIFICATION  AS  EGO-INVOLVEMENTS       139 

in  other  colleges,  there  was  a  clear  indication  that  those  who 
thought  they  were  being  compared  with  whites  had  considerably 
less  confidence  in  themselves  with  respect  either  to  the  mainte- 
nance of  past  performance  or  the  attainment  of  higher  scores  in 
the  future.  The  ego-disturbance  or  ego-insecurity  found  here 
among  those  Negroes  who  believed  their  past  performance  was 
equal  to  that  of  a  group  generally  regarded  as  "superior"  is  com- 
parable to  the  feeling  of  inferiority  so  commonly  found  in  every- 
day life  among  those  who,  objectively,  are  as  able,  as  good-looking, 
or  as  successful  as  certain  other  individuals  with  whom  they  com- 
pare themselves  but  who,  in  their  eyes,  seem  superior  because  of 
various  status  affiliations.  In  a  complementary  study,  Macintosh 
[47]  found  that  when  white  subjects  compared  themselves  with 
the  hypothetical  scores  of  Negroes  they  tended  to  raise  their  own 
estimates  and  showed  greater  confidence  in  their  own  abilities. 

Psychologists  have  only  recently  begun  to  study  the  problem  of 
class  identification  in  contemporary  America.  We  have  already 
referred  to  Kornhauser's  pioneering  attempt  to  compare  the  atti- 
tudes of  individuals  whose  class  placement  might  be  inferred  from 
the  amount  and  source  of  their  income  or  from  their  occupation 
(p.  75).  Certain  differences  between  the  attitudes  of  members 
of  the  upper,  middle,  or  lower  class,  as  defined  by  objective  cri- 
teria, did  appear  with  respect  to  some  social  and  political  issues. 
But  differences  did  not  show  up  with  reference  to  those  attitudes 
which  reflected  in  the  1930's  the  traditional  American  ideology  of 
individual  opportunity. 

In  Cantril's  study  [9],  based  on  a  nationwide  survey  made  in 
1941,  a  sample  of  the  American  adult  population  was  asked  the 
following  two  direct  questions:  "Which  income  group  in  our 
country  do  you  feel  that  you  are  a  member  of — the  middle  income 
group,  the  upper  income  group,  or  the  lower  income  group?"  and 
"To  what  social  class  in  this  country  do  you  feel  you  belong — 
middle  class,  or  upper,  or  lower?"  Only  3  per  cent  of  over  3,000 
people  interviewed  were  unable  to  place  themselves  in  some  social 
class,  and  only  1  per  cent  could  not  fit  themselves  into  an  economic 
class.  That  these  questions  were  meaningful  is  further  indicated 
by  the  reports  of  the  interviewers  who  stated  that  they  had  no 
difficulty  at  all  in  getting  the  answers  to  the  questions. 


140  EXPERIMENTS  ON  EGO-INVOLVEMENTS 

The  results  showed  that  at  that  time,  and  with  only  these  three 
alternatives  provided  for  answers,  almost  nine  tenths  of  the  Ameri- 
can people  identified  themselves  with  the  great  middle  class.   This 
middle  class  tradition  and  ideology  has  its  roots  in  frontier  in- 
dividualism and  in  the  relatively  high  standard  of  living  provided 
in  this  country  by  natural  resources  and  technological  develop- 
ments and  regarded  as  a  middle  class  standard.   Even  70  per  cent 
of  those  who  feel  they  are  members  of  the  lower  income  group 
still  call  themselves  members  of  the  middle  social  group.    There 
is  a  strict  correspondence  between  the  two  class  identifications — 
social  and  economic — among  only  54  per  cent  of  the  representa- 
tive population.   As  Veblen  pointed  out  so  strikingly  in  his  Theory 
of  the  Leisure  Class,  there  is  a  definite  tendency  for  individuals  to 
regard  their  social  class  within  a  frame  of  reference  provided  by 
the  norms  of  their  social  system  and  frequently  unsupported  by 
the  income  necessary  to  solidify  their  own  positions  objectively  in 
the  social  level  they  accept  as  their  own.    The  greatest  disparity 
between  income  and  social  identification  is  found  among  the  low 
income  groups,  while  those  who  placed  themselves  higher  in  the 
social  scale  tended  to  base  their  identification  less  on  strictly  eco- 
nomic criteria,  using  in  addition  to  income  such  commonly  ac- 
cepted values  as  family  background,  education,  and  professional 
accomplishments.    It  is  clear  from  this  preliminary  study  that 
nearly  every  adult  American  easily  thinks  of  himself  as  a  member 
of  a  class,  that  the  great  majority  of  Americans  at  that  time  rather 
uncritically  identified  themselves  with  the  middle  class  which  has 
for  so  long  been  the  traditional  bulwark  of  American  ideology. 
Because  of  this  middle  class  identification  it  is  not  surprising  that 
Kornhauser  found  many  inconsistencies  in  the  attitudes  people 
"logically"  should  have  as  members  of  different  economic  or  occu- 
pational groups  and  the  attitudes  they  actually  do  have. 

The  most  thorough  psychological  analysis  to  date  of  class  struc- 
ture in  America  is  that  of  Centers  [12].  He  carefully  designed 
a  questionnaire  which  would  reveal  subjective  class  identification, 
attitudes  toward  major  economic  and  social  issues,  adherence  to 
certain  traditional  conceptions  of  American  ideology  (for  example, 
that  this  is  a  land  of  freedom  and  opportunity),  occupation,  re- 
ligious affiliation,  nationality  background,  and  the  like.  The  ques- 


STATUS  AND  CLASS  IDENTIFICATION  AS  EGO-INVOLVEMENTS       141 

tionnaire  was  used  in  a  public  opinion  survey  in  the  summer  of 
1945.  Approximately  1,100  representative  adult  white  men  in  the 
United  States  were  personally  interviewed.  It  should  be  remem- 
bered that  Centers'  survey  was  made  before  the  end  of  World 
War  II,  that  is,  while  both  labor  and  management  were  still  by 
and  large  submerging  their  differences  in  the  interests  of  the  war 
effort. 

The  question  used  by  Centers  to  reveal  subjective  class  identifi- 
cation included  the  additional  category  of  "working"  class.  This 
wording  makes  possible  a  more  accurate  and  objective  class  dif- 
ferentiation (and  one  apparently  understandable  by  the  popula- 
tion) in  terms  of  relationship  to  the  processes  of  production  and 
exchange  in  contemporary  society.4  Centers'  over-all  national  re- 
sults on  class  identification  are  as  follows,  to  the  question:  "If  you 
were  asked  to  use  one  of  these  four  names  for  your  social  class, 
which  would  you  say  you  belonged  in:  the  middle  class,  lower 
class,  working  class,  or  upper  class?" 

Upper  3% 

Middle  43 

Working  51 

Lower  1 

Don't  know  1 

Don't  believe  in  classes  1 

This  subjective  identification  with  one  of  the  four  classes  de- 
scribed correlates  highly  with  an  individual's  economic  status  as 
judged  by  the  interviewer  (see  Table  1).  It  is  possible,  then,  to 
make  a  prediction  of  some  reliability  concerning  class  identifica- 
tion from  an  individual's  income  or  standard  of  living.  That 
those  who  identify  themselves  with  the  working  class  realize  the 

4  The  difference  between  Centers'  findings  and  those  already  reported  by  Cantril, 
as  well  as  other  results  of  the  same  type  found  in  surveys  of  the  American 
Institute  of  Public  Opinion  and  the  Fortune  poll  provide  an  excellent  example  of 
the  effect  of  the  wording  of  questions  on  results  obtained  in  public  opinion  surveys 
and  the  consequent  need  for  caution  in  interpretation  until  issues  or  groupings 
have  been  variously  sliced.  In  Centers'  survey  for  example  (had  sufficient  funds 
been  available)  it  would  have  been  revealing  to  substitute  on  a  comparable  sample 
of  the  population  the  word  "laboring"  for  "working"  class.  Other  obvious  pos- 
sibilities also  come  to  mind — the  use  of  categories  such  as  "management,"  "white 
collar,"  "professional." 


142  EXPERIMENTS  ON  EGO-INVOLVEMENTS 

relative  uncertainty  and  precariousness  of  their  jobs  is  revealed  by 
Centers'  finding  that  when  asked  which  kind  of  job  they  would 
choose,  those  who  put  themselves  in  the  working  class  gave  as 
their  most  frequent  reply  "a  job  which  you  were  absolutely  sure 
of  keeping,"  whereas  those  who  put  themselves  in  the  middle  class 
gave  as  their  most  frequent  reply  "a  job  where  you  could  express 
your  feelings,  ideas,  talent,  or  skill." 

TABLE  i 

[CLASS  IDENTIFICATION  BY  ECONOMIC  STATUS 


Don't 

Don't  Believe 

Economic  Status 

Upper 

Middle 

Working 

Lower 

Know 

in  Class 

Total 

Above  average 

9% 

75% 

14% 

0.5% 

1% 

0.5% 

100% 

Avcracc 

2 

58 

38 

1 

1 

100 

4  X  V  Wl  <*£W 

Below  avcracc 

1 

23 

72 

2 

2 

100 

Particularly  striking  is  the  relationship  found  by  Centers  be- 
tween subjective  class  identification  and  objective  status  as  defined 
by  occupation.  As  Chart  3  shows,  approximately  three  quarters  of 
all  manual  workers,  whether  skilled,  semiskilled,  or  unskilled,  feel 
they  belong  to  the  wording  class.5  On  the  other  hand  approxi- 
mately the  same  proportion  of  business  owners  and  managers  and 
of  professional  people  put  themselves  in  the  middle  class.  White 
collar  workers,  farm  owners,  and  managers  appear  to  have  a 
somewhat  more  ambivalent  position.  The  chart  shows  beyond 
any  shadow  of  doubt  that  the  American  people  do  identify  them- 
selves with  a  social  class  and  that  this  identification  is  highly  cor- 
related with  the  particular  role  they  play  in  our  highly  indus- 
trialized society. 

A  special  study  of  foremen  in  American  factories  made  by  the 
Opinion  Research  Corporation  in  1945  [51]  shows  that  the  ma- 
jority of  them  identify  themselves  with  management  rather  than 
with  the  workers.  Furthermore,  of  particular  significance  is  the 
fact  that,  the  longer  the  foremanship  tenure,  the  greater  is  the 
tendency  to  identify  with  management  rather  than  with  labor. 
The  study  also  shows  that  foremen  who  were  former  union  mem- 
bers identify  themselves  with  management  almost  as  much  as 

5  These  charts  were  designed  by  Richard  Centers. 


STATUS  AND  CLASS  IDENTIFICATION  AS  EGO-INVOLVEMENTS       H3 


3      = 


CO 

i 


144  EXPERIMENTS  ON  EGO-INVOLVEMENTS 

those  who  had  not  been  union  members  (57  per  cent  compared 
to  62  per  cent).  Foremen  clearly  occupy  an  ambivalent  member- 
ship position  in  our  social  organization.  What  identification  they 
may  once  have  had  with  workers  tends  to  disappear;  their  loyalties 
become  relatively  mixed;  they  are  not  part  of  management,  yet 
they  tend  to  identify  themselves  with  those  in  the  "top"  positions 
of  authority  and  control.6  Table  2  shows  the  answers  given  by 
764  foremen  to  the  question:  "There  are  several  views  as  to  the 
place  that  foremen  occupy  in  relation  to  management  and  the 

TABLE  2 
FOREMAN  IDENTIFICATION  BY  LENGTH  OF  FORBMANSHIP  TENURE 


Neither, 

Foremanskip  Tenure 

Casts 

Management 

Workers 

in  Between 

Don't  Know 

Under  2  years 

227 

49% 

22% 

26% 

3% 

2-5  years 

169 

61 

18 

20 

1 

Over  5  years 

365 

64 

19 

16 

1 

No  answer 

3 

Total  764          59%  20%          20%  1% 

workers.   Would  you  say  that  foremen  are  a  part  of  management, 
or  that  they  are  more  like  workers?" 

The  various  occupational  groups  that  members  of  the  working 
and  middle  classes  assign  to  their  own  class  in  Centers'  study  are 
shown  in  Charts  4  and  5.  Charts  6  and  7  indicate  some  very 
significant  aspects  of  class  identification.  Here  there  is  a  compari- 
son of  the  definitions  given  to  the  two  major  classes,  middle  and 
working,  by  each  of  two  objectively  different  occupational  strata. 
A  striking  fact  is  that  though  people  may  differ  greatly  in  their 
objective  status  they  define  the  class  with  which  they  identify 
themselves  in  essentially  similar  fashion.  Middle  class  and  work- 
ing class  apparently  means  about  the  same  thing  to  one  stratum  as 
to  another.  Yet  it  is  noteworthy  that  the  discrepancies  that  do 
occur  in  the  contrasting  conceptions  of  a  given  class  are  systemati- 
cally biased  in  favor  of  inclusion  in  the  class  of  "members"  of  the 
individual's  own  occupational  group. 

°For  a  more  detailed  discussion  of  the  status  of  foremen  see  [22]. 


STATUS  AND  CLASS  IDENTIFICATION  AS  EGO-INVOLVEMENTS       145 

As  would  be  expected,  reliable  differences  appear  between  the 
attitudes  of  working  class  and  middle  class  members  with  respect 
to  attitudes  concerned  with  basic  socioeconomic  issues.  Centers 
constructed  a  scale  of  radicalism-conservatism  from  six  separate 
questions  (such  as  giving  the  working  people  more  power  and 
influence  in  government,  government  ownership  of  industry, 
sympathy  with  workers  versus  employers  in  strikes).  As  Table  3 
indicates,  those  who  are  middle  class  are  considerably  more  con- 
servative than  the  working  class.  Differences  of  opinion  between 

TABLE  3 

CLASS  IDENTIFICATION  BY  RADICALISM-CONSERVATISM 
Class  Identification 


Attitude  Position 
Radical 
Conservative 
Indeterminate 


Middle 

Working 

Per 

Per 

cent 

Cases 

cent 

Cases 

12 

55 

31 

177 

67 

314 

36 

201 

21 

98 

33 

186 

100  467  100  564 


the  working  and  middle  class  found  by  Centers  are  probably  con- 
servative, in  view  of  the  facts  that  most  public  opinion  interviewers 
are  themselves  members  of  the  middle  class,  and  we  know  from 
Katz'  study  [38]  opinions  reported  by  working  class  interviewers 
tended  to  be  definitely  more  radical  than  those  reported  by  middle 
class  interviewers  on  working  people. 

It  is  significant  that,  although  the  percentage  of  working  class 
people  who  are  defined  in  Centers'  scale  as  conservatives  is  only 
about  half  as  great  as  those  defined  as  conservative  in  the  middle 
class,  there  are  still  more  working  class  people  in  the  conservative 
than  in  the  radical  group.  This  demonstrates  that  class  conscious- 
ness does  not  rapidly  and  spontaneously  arise.  It  requires  leader- 
ship and  organization  to  transform  a  loose  numerical  class  into  a 
compact  psychological  class.  It  is  therefore  important  to  bear  in 
mind  the  distinction  between  objective  and  subjective  class  differ- 
entiation. As  of  1945,  although  a  plurality  of  American  men 
identified  themselves  with  the  working  class,  they  can  by  no  means 


146 


EXPERIMENTS  ON  EGO-INVOLVEMENTS 


be  regarded  as  highly  "class-conscious."  A  breakdown  of  radi- 
calism-conservatism by  occupational  groups  shows,  however,  a 
clear  tendency  for  conservatism  to  decrease  and  radicalism  to  in- 
crease as  one  goes  "down"  the  occupational  ladder  (Chart  8).  It 


Big  business  ownership 
and  management 


Doctors  and  lawyers 


Small  business  ownership 
and  management 


Store  and  factory  managers 


Salesmen 


Office  workers 


Farmers 


Factory  workers 


Laborers 


Waiters  and  bartenders 


Servants 


0-2% 

38% 


|  48% 


?l&  82% 


53% 


I  55% 


CHART  4.    Occupational  composition  of  the  working  class  based  on  working-class 
specifications  of  the  occupational  membership. 

is  also  noteworthy  that  the  size  of  the  undecided  category  increases 
as  occupational  status  is  lowered.  Centers'  data  further  show  that 
within  each  occupational  category,  those  who  call  themselves 
members  of  the  working  class  are  less  conservative  than  those  who 
identify  themselves  with  the  middle  class. 

In  brief,  we  have  in  Centers'  study  a  demonstration  of  the  fact 
that  individuals  in  contemporary  America  do  identify  themselves 


SOME  RECENT  REVIEWS  OF  EXPERIMENTAL  LITERATURE 


147 


with  a  social  class,  that  this  identification  is  largely  determined  by 
the  objective  relation  or  status  they  have  with  respect  to  the  means 
of  production  and  exchange  in  an  industrial  society,  and  that  this 
identification  tends  to  be  accompanied  by  characteristic  attitudes 


Big  business  ownership 
and  management 

Doctors  and  lawyers 


Small  business  ownership 
and  management 


Store  and  factory  managers 


Salesmen 


Office  workers 


Farmers 


Factory  workers 


Laborers 


Waiters  and  bartenders 


Servants 


13% 


I  40% 


1 81% 


163% 


154% 


153% 


24% 


18% 


18% 


CHART  5.    Occupational  composition  of  the  middle  class  according  to  the  speci- 
fications of  the  middle  class. 

related  to  the  social  organization  which  has  imposed  on  people 
the  particular  role  they  have  within  it. 


SOME  RECENT  REVIEWS  OF  EXPERIMENTAL  LITERATURE 

In  order  to  make  our  position  quite  clear,  we  cite  some  examples 
of  the  conclusions  drawn  by  others  who  have  recently  reviewed 
the  experimental  literature  on  ego-involvement. 


148 


EXPERIMENTS  ON  EGO-INVOLVEMENTS 


In  his  discussion  of  The  Ego  in  Contemporary  Psychology 
(1943),  G.  W.  Allport  [1]  brought  together  much  of  the  recent 


100 


90 


80 


70 


60 


50 


40 


30 


20 


10 


I         I 

I         i      .9- 

i-     i    f . 


1 

—  . 

i 

\ 

y 

/ 

\ 

/ 

^ 

\ 

Business/  professional,  '    / 
and  white  collar    -W 
working  class  definition  / 

\ 

f 

\ 

/ 

/ 

/ 

/ 

\ 

/ 
/ 

/  Manual  workers 
/*-  working  class 
/          definition 

/ 

/ 

* 

v 

"*"-^ 

/ 

/- 

' 

!  !     I 


.«>£E         B^i^^^ls 

CD  Q        00  0700Ol2l2-35<7» 

CHART  6.    Comparison  of  two  definitions  of  working  class. 

experimental  work  and  reaffirmed  the  validity  of  the  concept  of 
ego  for  modern  psychology.  But  Allport's  eclectic  approach  seems 
to  us  to  have  somewhat  obscured  the  systematic  psychological 


SOME  RECENT  REVIEWS  OF  EXPERIMENTAL  LITERATURE 


149 


meaning  the  term  should  have.    He  distinguished  eight  differ* 
ent  conceptions  of  the  ego  as  held  by  psychologists  and  believes 


100 


90 


70 
60 
50 
40 
30 
20 
10 
0 


/   I  /  Manual  workers 
/    IK  middle  class 
/    I/       definition 


Business,  professional, 

and  white  collar 
middle  class  definition 


CHART  7.    Comparison  of  two  definitions  of  middle  class. 

that  there  is  experimental  support  for  all  eight  of  the  capacities 
of  the  ego  he  reviewed,  even  though  among  the  eight  are  such 
divergent  and  conflicting  characterizations  as  "the  ego  as  primitive 


150 


EXPERIMENTS  ON  EGO-INVOLVEMENTS 


selfishness,"  "the  ego  as  dominance-drive,"  "the  ego  as  a  passive 
organization  of  mental  processes,"  and  "the  ego  as  the  subjective 
organization  of  culture."  In  criticizing  our  earlier  discussion  of 
the  ego,  he  contended  that  the  ego  for  us  is  "nothing  but  'the 
social  in  man.' "  [458]  Although  we  have  always  maintained  and 


Large  business  ownership 
and  management 


Professional 


Small  business  ownership 
and  management 


8796 


11%   296 


71% 


19%  10% 


7596 


1796  896 


5496 


3896 


35% 


2196 


White  collar  workers 
Skilled  manual  workers 
Semiskilled  manual  workers 

Unskilled  manual  workers      

Legend 
Conservative 
Indeterminate 
••  Radical 

CHART  8.    Radicalism-conservatism  by  occupation. 

still  do  maintain  that  social  values  constitute  the  major  portion  of 
the  normal  individual's  ego,  we  have  not  equated  the  ego  with 
nothing  but  social  values.  We  have  pointed  out  in  previous 
publications  that  what  can  be  described  as  "personal"  values,  as 
contrasted  with  "social"  values  can  and  often  do  constitute  an 
important  part  of  the  egos  of  different  individuals. 

For  example,  Sherif,  in  1936,  described  personal  fixations  and 
evaluations.  [60, 120  ff.]  In  his  summary  discussion  of  the  ego  he 
stated: 

Values  are  the  chief  constituents  of  the  ego.    Among  these,  social 
values,  which  are  socially  established  affective  fixations,  form  the 


SOME  RECENT  REVIEWS  OF  EXPERIMENTAL  LITERATURE  151 

major  (directive)  part.    These  values  are  the  social  in  man.  [60, 185, 
italics  not  in  original] 

Cantril  [7]  wrote  in  1940  that  the  "ego  of  an  individual  is  essen- 
tially composed  of  the  many  social  and  personal  values  he  has 
accepted."  [197 /.]  And  again,  in  1941  [8],  that 

Although  we  do  conform  in  large  measure  to  the  common  values 
of  society  and  do  desire  a  certain  amount  of  social  recognition,  in 
addition  to  this — and  often  much  more  important  in  terms  of  felt 
significance — we  cherish  certain  values  which  may  be  shared  by  our 
family  circle,  a  few  professional  colleagues,  a  local  community  group, 
or  a  political  party.  And  sometimes  we  cherish,  as  most  important  of 
all,  those  values  which  we  have  worked  out  for  ourselves  and  thin\  of 
as  our  own.  ...  In  such  instances,  we  care  about  recognition  only 
from  a  very  few  people,  or  the  status  we  may  want  is  status  only  in 
our  own  eyes.  [8,  45f  italics  not  in  original  statements] 

Although  Allport  is  critical  of  the  interpretation  of  the  "ego  as 
the  subjective  organization  of  culture,"  still,  in  the  last  analysis, 
the  weight  of  evidence  brings  him  close  to  our  position.  He 
writes,  for  example,  that  "what  an  individual  regards  as  himself 
is  undeniably,  in  large  part,  socially  determined."  [1,  458]  He 
states  further  that  for  the  normal  individual  the  "ego-system  is 
made  up  of  the  ordinary  values  which  spell  out  the  significance 
of  life  to  the  individual."  [470] 

Allport  states  further  that  values  which  cannot  be  characterized 
as  "social"  must  be  called  "egoistic." 

If  the  ego  is  nothing  but  the  "social  in  man,"  one  wonders  what  to 
call  all  the  anti-social  impulses  and  the  solitary  strivings  that  are  nor- 
mally called  egoistic?  [1,  458] 

This  is  more  an  ethical  than  a  psychological  characterization.  And 
a  mixture  of  ethics  and  psychology  makes  it  difficult  for  psycholo- 
gists who  want  to  use  the  term  "ego"  in  a  precise  psychological 
way  to  show  their  colleagues  they  are  not  trying  once  more  to 
bring  into  the  discipline  some  modern  version  of  the  soul.  The 
tendency  to  mix  psychology  and  ethics,  to  relate  the  "self  in  some 
way  to  "selfish"  interests,  was  criticized  by  Baldwin  and  Cooley 


152  EXPERIMENTS  ON  EGO-INVOLVEMENTS 

around  the  turn  of  the  century.  Baldwin  felt  that  any  attempted 
equation  was  "quite  beside  the  mark.'*  [5]  Cooley  pointed  out 
in  1902  that 

Self  and  other  do  not  exist  as  mutually  exclusive  social  facts,  and 
phraseology  which  implies  that  they  do,  like  the  antithesis  egoism 
versus  altruism,  is  open  to  the  objection  of  vagueness,  if  not  of  falsity. 
It  seems  to  me  that  the  classification  of  impulses  as  altruistic  and 
egoistic,  with  or  without  a  third  class  called,  perhaps,  ego-altruistic,  is 
empty;  and  I  do  not  see  how  any  other  conclusion  can  result  from  a 
concrete  study  of  the  matter.  [15, 126  f.] 

Lewis  also  defines  "ego"  needs  as  "selfish"  needs  [44].  But  she 
found  from  her  own  experiments  on  co-operative  effort  that  such 
a  definition  was  too  limited,  that  subjects  working  together  identi- 
fied themselves  with  one  another  so  that  each  got  satisfaction  if 
the  job  at  hand  was  satisfactorily  completed,  whether  by  one's 
self  or  one's  partner.  To  interpret  this  finding,  as  Lewis  has,  as 
"task  orientation"  rather  than  "ego  orientation"  overlooks  and  con^ 
tradicts  her  own  statements  that  in  these  co-operative  situations 
"  'ego-boundaries'  may  include  the  needs  of  other  selves  or  'egos,' 
of  groups,  of  ideals."  [45,  214}  Her  experiment  demonstrates 
nicely  that  there  can  be  and  is  ego-involvement  in  co-operative 
effort  as  well  as  in  restricted  individualistic  pursuits, 

A  somewhat  more  recent  review  of  experimental  literature  by 
Holt  (1945)  [30]  shows  the  confusion  that  results  from  the  at- 
tempt to  get  away  from  a  formulation  merely  by  creating  a  new 
terminology.  Holt  maintains  that  the  common  denominator  of 
most  meanings  of  ego-involvements  is  "self-esteem."  But  he  goes 
on  to  say  that  he  cannot  use  the  term  "self-esteem  involvement" 
because  it  is  too  narrow  to  define  all  the  functions  that  "comprise 
the  ego." 

In  summary,  the  experiments  and  controlled  investigations  re- 
ported all  add  up  to  substantiate  further  the  position  that  the  ego 
is  a  genetic  formation  made  up  of  a  host  of  personal  and  social 
values  and  that  these  values  serve  the  individual  as  frames  of  ref- 
erence by  means  of  which  he  makes  those  judgments  that  affect 
him;  that  define  for  him  success  and  failure;  that  determine  his 
loyalties  and  allegiances;  that  spell  out  what  he  conceives  to  be  his 


REFERENCES  153 

role,  his  status,  his  class.  Judgments  and  behavior  resulting  from 
this  identification  of  oneself  with  a  certain  constellation  of  values 
we  can  properly  term  "ego-involved."  "Ego-involvement"  is  a 
general  descriptive  term  that  can  have  many  specific  and  more 
precise  meanings  (such  as  "ego-enhancement,"  "ego-gratification," 
"ego-frustration,"  "ego-support,"  "ego-misplacement,"  "ego-expan- 
sion," "ego-breakdown"),  depending  on  the  particular  set  of 
circumstances  in  which  the  ego  is  involved. 

REFERENCES 

1.  ALLPORT,  G.  W.,  The  ego  in  contemporary  psychology,  Psychol.  Rev.,  1943, 

50,  451-78. 

2.  ALPER,  T.  G.,  Task-orientation  vs.  ego-orientation  in  learning  and  retention, 

Am.  J.  Psychol.,  1946,  59,  236-48. 

3.  ANDERSON,  H.  H.,  and  H.  F.  BRANDT,  A  study  of  motivation  involving  self- 

announced  goals  of  fifth  grade  children  and  the  concept  of  level  of  aspira- 
tion, /.  Soc.  Psychol.,  1939,  10,  209-32. 

4.  ASCH,  S.  E.,  H.  BLOCK,  and  M.  HERTZMAN,  Studies  in  the  principles  of  judg- 

ments and  attitudes:  I.  Two  basic  principles  of  judgment,  /.  Psychol.,  1938, 
5,  219-51. 

5.  BALDWIN,  J.  M.,  Social  and  Ethical  Interpretations  in  Mental  Development, 

New  York:  Macmillan,  copyright  1897. 

6.  BIRCH,  H.  G.,  The  effect  of  socially  disapproved  labeling  upon  a  well-structured 

attitude,  /.  Abnorm.  6-  Soc.  Psychol.,  1945,  40,  301-10. 

7.  CANTRIL,  H.,  The  Invasion  from  Mars,  Princeton:  Univ.  Press,  1940. 

8.  The  Psychology  of  Social  Movements,  New  York:  Wiley,  copyright  1941. 

9.  Identification  with  social  and  economic  class,  /.  Abnorm.  &  Soc.  Psychol., 

1943,  38,  74-80. 

10.  The  use  of  breakdowns,  in  Gauging  Public  Opinion  (H.  Cantril,  ed.), 

Princeton:  Univ.  Press,  1944,  ch.  13. 
11. The  intensity  of  an  attitude,  /  Abnorm.  &  Soc.  Psychol.,  1946,  41,  129- 

35. 

12.  CENTERS,  R.,  Psychological  Aspects  of  Socio-economic  Stratification:  an  Enquiry 

into  the  Nature  of  Class,  Princeton  Univ.,  to  be  published. 

13.  CHAPMAN,  D.  W.,  and  J.  VOLKMANN,  A  social  determinant  of  the  level  of  aspi- 

ration, /.  Abnorm.  6-  Soc.  Psychol.,  1939,  34,  225-38. 

14.  CLAPAREDE,  E.,  Recognition  et  Moiite,  Arch,  de  Psych.,  1911,  11. 

15.  COOLEY,  C.  H.,  Human  Nature  and  the  Social  Order,  New  York:  Scribner's, 

copyright  1902. 

16.  DAVIS,  J.,  Testing  the  social  attitudes  of  children  in  the  government  schools  in 

Russia,  Am.  /.  Sociol,  1927,  32,  947-52. 

17.  EDWARDS,  A.  L.,  Political  frames  of  reference  as  a  factor  influencing  recognition, 

/.  Abnorm.  &  Soc.  Psychol.,  1941,  36,  34-50. 

18. Rationalization  in  recognition  as  a  result  of  a  political  frame  of  reference, 

/.  Abnorm.  &  Soc.  Psychol,  1941,  36,  224-35. 


154  EXPERIMENTS  ON  EGO-INVOLVEMENTS 

19.  FRANK,  J.  D.,  The  influence  of  the  level  of  performance  in  one  task  on  the  level 
of  aspiration  in  another,  /.  Exp.  PsychoL,  1935,  18,  159-71. 

20. Some  psychological  determinants  of  the  level  of  aspiration,  Am.  J.  Psy- 

chol,  1935,  47,  285-93. 

21. Recent  studies  of  the  level  of  aspiration,  PsychoL  Bull,  1941,  38,  218-26. 

22.  GOLDEN,  C.  S.,  and  H.  J.  RUTTENBERG,  The  Dynamics  of  Industrial  Democracy, 

New  York:  Harper,  1942. 

23.  GOULD,  R.,  An  experimental  analysis  of  "level  of  aspiration,"  Genet.  PsychoL 

Monogr.,  1939,21,  1-116. 

24. and  H,  B.  LEWIS,  An  experimental  investigation  of  changes  in  the  mean- 
ing of  level  of  aspiration,  /.  Exp.  PsychoL,  1940,  27,  422-38. 

25.  GREENBERG,  P.  J.,  Competition  in  children,  Am.  J.  PsychoL,  1932,  44,  22 1-48. 

26.  HEATHERS,  L.  B.,  Factors  producing  generality  in  the  level  of  aspiration,  /.  Exp. 

PsychoL,  1942,  30,  392-406. 

27.  HERTZMAN,  M.,  and  L.  FESTINGER,  Shifts  in  explicit  goals  in  a  level  of  aspira- 

tion experiment,  /.  Exp.  PsychoL,  1940,  27,  439-52. 

28.  HILGARD,  E.  R.,  E.  M.  SAIT,  and  G.  A.  MAGARET,  Level  of  aspiration  as  affected 

by  relative  standing  in  an  experimental  social  group,  /.  Exp.  PsychoL,  1940, 
27,411-21. 

29. The  enigma  of  Japanese  friendliness,  Publ.   Opin.   Quart.,   1946,   10, 

343-8. 

30.  HOLT,  R.  R.,  Effects  of  ego-involvement  upon  levels  of  aspiration,  Psychiatry: 

J.  BioL  &  Path.  Interpers.  Relations,  1945,  3,  299-317. 

31.  HOPPE,  F.,  Erfolg  und  Misserfolg,  PsychoL  Forsch.,  1930,  14,  1-62. 

32.  HORNEY,  K.,  New  Ways  in  Psychoanalysis,  New  York:  Norton,  copyright  1939. 

33.  HUNTLEY,  C.  W.,  Judgments  of  self  based  upon  records  of  expressive  behavior, 

/.  Abnorm.  &  Soc.  PsychoL,  1940,  35,  398-427. 

34.  HYMAN,  H.  H.,  The  psychology  of  status,  Arch.  PsychoL,  1942,  no.  269. 

35.  JAMES,  W.,  The  Principles  of  Psychology,  New  York:  Holt,  copyright  1890. 

36.  JOHNSON,  C.  S.,  Growing  Up  in  the  BlacJ^  Belt,  Washington:  Am.  Council  on 

Education,  1941. 

37.  KARSTEN,  A.,  Psychische  Sattigung,  Psych.  Forsch.,  1928,  10,  142-254. 

38.  KATZ,  D.,  Do  interviewers  bias  poll  results?,  Publ.  Opin.  Quart.,  1942,  6, 

248-68. 

39.  The  measurement  of  intensity,  in  Gauging  Public  Opinion  (H.  Cantril, 

ed.),  Princeton:  Univ.  Press,  1944,  ch.  3. 

40.  KAY,  L.  W.,  The  relation  of  personal  frames  of  reference  to  social  judgments, 

Arch.  PsychoL,  1943,  no.  283. 

41.  KLEIN,  G.  S.,  and  N.  SCHOENFELD,  The  influence  of  ego-involvement  on  con- 

fidence, /.  Abnorm.  &  Soc.  PsychoL,  1941,  36,  249-58. 

42.  KOFFKA,  K.,  Principles  of  Gestalt  Psychology,  New  York:  Harcourt,  Brace, 

copyright  1935. 

43.  LEVINE,  J.  M.,  and  G.  MURPHY,  The  learning  and  forgetting  of  controversial 

material,  /.  Abnorm.  &  Soc.  PsychoL,  1943,  38,  507-17. 

44.  LEWIS,  H.  B.,  An  experimental  study  of  the  role  of  the  ego  in  work:  I.  The  role 

of  the  ego  in  cooperative  work,  /.  Exp.  PsychoL,  1944,  34,  113-26. 

45.  : and  M.  FRANKLIN,  An  experimental  study  of  the  role  of  the  ego  in  work: 

II.  The  significance  of  task-orientation  in  work,  /.  Exp.  PsychoL,  1944,  34, 
195-215. 


REFERENCES  155 

46.  MACCURDY,  J.  T.,  Common  Principles  in  Psychology  and  Physiology,  Cambridge 

(England) :  Univ.  Press,  1928. 

47.  MACINTOSH,  A.,  Differential  effect  of  the  status  of  the  competing  group  upon 

the  levels  of  aspiration,  Am.  J.  PsychoL,  1942,  55,  546-54. 

48.  MARKS,  E.,  Skin  color  judgments  of  Negro  college  students,  /.  Abnorm.  6* 

Soc.  PsychoL,  1943,  38,  370-6. 

49.  McGEHEE,  W.,  Judgment  and  the  level  of  aspiration,  /.  Gen.  PsychoL,  1940,  22, 

3-15. 

50.  MURPHY,  G.,  L.  B.  MURPHY,  and  T.  M.  NEWCOMB,  Experimental  Social  Psy- 

chology, New  York:  Harper,  1937. 

51.  OPINION  RESEARCH  CORPORATION,  Foremen  Thinking  Today,  a  .survey  made  by 

the  Public  Opinion  Index  for  Industry,  March  1946. 

52.  OSGOOD,  C.  E.,  and  R.  STAGNER,  Analysis  of  a  prestige  frame  of  reference  by  a 

gradient  technique,  /.  Appl  PsychoL,  1941,  25,  275-90. 

53.  POSTMAN,  L.,  arid  C.  ZIMMERMAN,  Intensity  of  attitude  as  a  determinant  of 

decision  time,  Am.  /.  PsychoL,  1945,  58,  510-18. 

54.  PRESTON,  M.  G.,  and  J.  A.  BAYTON,  Differential  effect  of  a  social  variable  upon 

three  levels  of  aspiration,  /.  Exp.  PsychoL,  1941,  29,  351-69. 

55.  ROSENZWEIG,  S.,  Preferences  in  the  repetition  of  successful  and  unsuccessful 

activities  as  a  function  of  age  and  personality,  /.  Genet.  PsychoL,  1933,  42, 
423-41. 

56. An  experimental  study  of  "Repression"  with  special  reference  to  need- 
persistent  and  ego-defensive  reactions  to  frustration,  /.  Exp.  PsychoL,  1943, 
32,  64-74. 

57.  SEARS,  P.  S.,  Levels  of  aspiration  in  academically  successful  and  unsuccessful 

children,  /.  Abnorm.  &  Soc.  PsychoL,  1940,  35,  498-536. 

58.  SEELEMAN,  V.,  The  influence  of  attitude  upon  the  remembering  of  pictorial 

material,  Arch.  Psychol.,  1940,  no.  258. 

59.  SHAW,  F.  }.,  and  A.  SPOONER,  Selective  forgetting  when  the  subject  is  not  "ego- 

involved,"  /.  Exp.  PsychoL,  1945,  35,  242-7. 

60.  SHERIF,  M.,  The  Psychology  of  Social  Norms,  New  York:  Harper,  copyright 

1936. 

61.  STOUFFER,  S.,  and  associates,  Experiments  on  the  measurement  of  intensity 

function  and  zero-point  in  attitude  analysis,  Headquarters,  Army  Service 
Forces,  Information  and  Education  Division,  Report  No.  D-l. 

62.  TURNBULL,  W.,  Secret  vs.  nonsecret  ballots,  in  Gauging  Public  Opinion  (H, 

Cantril,  ed.),  Princeton:  Univ.  Press,  1944,  ch.  5. 

63.  WALLEN,  R.  W.,  The  selective  forgetting  of  material  related  to  subjects'  esti- 

mates of  their  own  personality  traits,  Abstracts  of  Doctoral  Dissertations, 
1941,  no.  35. 

64. Ego-involvement  as  a  determinant  of  selective  forgetting,  /.  Abnorm.  & 

Soc.  PsychoL,  1942,  37,  20-39. 

65.  WOLFF,  W.,  Selbstbeurteilung  und  Fremdbeurteilung  im  wissentlichen  und  un- 

wissentlichen  Versuch,  PsychoL  Forsch.,  1932,  16,  251-328. 

66.  ZILLIG,  M.,  Einstellung  und  Aussage,  Z.  PsychoL,  1928,  106,  58-106. 


CHAPTER  7 
THE  GENETIC  FORMATION  OF  THE  EGO 

We  pointed  out  in  chapter  5  that  an  individual's  major  attitudes 
are  not  discrete  items  in  his  psychological  make-up.  Attitudes 
define  a  person's  identifications  and  status;  determine  to  an  im- 
portant degree  his  conformities  and  loyalties  to  his  surroundings, 
to  social  groups  and  institutions.  To  a  great  extent,  therefore,  a 
person's  experience  of  himself  is  related  to  these  attitudinal  ex- 
periences. In  other  words,  the  most  important  attitudes  formed 
in  relation  to  one's  body,  to  surrounding  objects,  persons,  institu- 
tions, and  groups,  are  ego-attitudes.  Inevitably  one  becomes  some- 
how ego-involved  when  his  intimate  friends,  his  superior,  his 
inferior,  his  family,  his  school,  his  church,  or  his  flag  are  in  ques- 
tion. Gratifications  and  frustrations  connected  with  such  persons, 
groups,  or  institutions  are  felt  as  ego-gratifications  and  ego-frus- 
trations. Attitudes  which  direct  a  person  to  try  to  outdo  others  in 
everything  (competitiveness)  or  to  live  and  work  in  harmony  as  a 
group  member  (co-operativeness)  are  ego-attitudes,  developed  by 
the  norms  of  the  social  system  in  which  he  lives.1  Whatever  we 
can  learn  about  these  ego-attitudes  should  contribute  to  a  real 
understanding  of  the  "human  nature"  about  which  social  scientists 
and  politicians  make  hasty  assumptions  in  an  effort  to  justify  their 
theories  and  practices. 

The  more  we  study,  the  more  we  find  that  the  ego  (the  self) 
consists  mainly  of  those  attitudes  formed  during  the  course  of 
genetic  development:  attitudes  related  to  one's  body,  parents, 
family,  school,  church,  profession,  property,  class,  and  the  like. 
As  attitudes  are  formed  (learned)  in  the  course  of  genetic  develop- 
ment, the  ego  is  formed.  Starting  with  the  delimitation  of  one's 

1  We  are  not  ignoring  the  fact  that  the  intensity  and  expression  of  attitudes  are, 
within  limits,  subject  to  individual  variations. 

156 


THE  GENETIC  FORMATION  OF  THE  EGO  157 

own  body  from  surrounding  objects,  ego  formation  rapidly  ex- 
pands by  the  learning  of  attitudes  related  to  it  (ego-attitudes), 
particularly  after  the  acquisition  of  communicable  language.  This 
is  repeatedly  verified  by  empirical  observations  of  child  develop- 
ment, from  the  earliest  to  the  most  recent  studies.  Ego  formation 
is  not  a  mystic  process.  It  can  be  readily  detected  in  the  behavior 
of  the  growing  child.  The  social  development  a  child  has  attained 
by  the  time  he  reaches  school  age  is  not  the  result  of  an  automatic 
blossoming  forth  of  some  inherent  natural  endowment.  In  view 
of  the  persistence  of  philosophical  speculations  concerning  the  ego, 
it  is  gratifying  to  find  this  verification  in  fact.  Indeed  the  psy- 
chology of  the  early  period  of  ego  formation  seems  to  be  one  of 
the  firmly  established  principles  in  the  whole  field  of  psychology. 
This  is  especially  significant  when  we  remember  that  the  relevant 
observations  were  collected  over  a  period  of  many  years  by  ob- 
servers who  were  geographically  separated  and  who  had  different 
theoretical  inclinations. 

If  this  fact  of  ego  formation,  so  well  verified  in  almost  every 
study  on  ego  development,  were  given  its  due  place  in  general 
books  on  child  psychology  and  if  its  important  implications  for  the 
process  of  socialization  of  the  child  were  pointed  out,  it  would  be 
unnecessary  for  us  to  review  here  the  ever-accumulating  evidence 
concerning  ego  formation  in  a  book  on  social  psychology.  But 
since  the  implications  have  not  been  systematically  brought  to- 
gether, we  shall  summarize  here  some  representative  studies,  from 
Tiedemann,  Preyer,  and  Shinn  to  those  of  present-day  psycholo- 
gists. In  reviewing  these  representative  observations  with  a  spe- 
cific purpose  in  mind,  we  shall  not  ignore  variations  of  the 
particular  components  of  a  child's  developing  ego  or  the  factors 
which  function  in  such  a  way  as  to  accelerate  or  retard  develop- 
ment. Briefly,  it  will  be  seen  that  both  the  components  of  the 
developing  ego  and  the  speed  of  its  development  will  vary  in  terms 
of  the  limitations  of  the  physical  surroundings:  the  kind  of  treat- 
ment (care,  resistances,  and  encouragements)  received  from  adults, 
opportunities  for  language  development,  contact  with  cultural 
products,  symbols,  norms,  associations  with  age  mates,  and  so  on. 
We  are  interested  in  the  fundamental  observations  which  show 


158  THE  GENETIC  FORMATION  OF  THE  EGO 

that  a  trend  in  development  does  occur.  And  we  shall  see  that,  in 
spite  of  variations,  the  general  trend  of  ego  development  revealed 
in  these  investigations  is  essentially  similar, 

A  REVIEW  OF  SOME  STUDIES  ON  EGO  DEVELOPMENT 

As  early  as  1787,  long  before  the  appearance  of  experimental 
psychology,  Dietrich  Tiedemann  [46]  published  his  observations 
on  his  son  from  birth  through  the  third  year.2  Many  of  his  reports 
are  relevant  to  the  problem  of  ego  formation.  When  the  child  was 
slightly  over  a  month  old,  Tiedemann  wrote  that 

...  the  boy  did  not  beat  or  scratch  himself  with  his  hands  as 
frequently  as  before;  so  it  seemed  that  painful,  oft  repeated  experience 
had  taught  him  to  draw  some  distinction  between  himself  and  foreign 
bodies.  [46,  210] 

One  year  and  four  months  later: 

His  power  of  judgment  was  even  more  clearly  evident  on  January 
11,  1783,  when  he  recognized  his  own  image  in  the  mirror.  .  .  .  Chil- 
dren are  often  held  up  to  the  mirror  from  earliest  infancy,  and  thus 
they  learn  by  repeated  experience  that  the  image  is  their  own.  [46, 
222] 

When  the  child  was  20  months  old,  we  find  a  picture  far  different 
from  that  of  the  baby  beating  his  own  body: 

On  the  27th  of  March  he  could  already  pronounce  words  of  two 
syllables,  and  knew  almost  all  the  external  parts  of  his  body,  which 
he  pointed  out  correctly  when  their  names  were  mentioned  to 
him;  .  .  .  [46,  225] 

In  the  same  year,  Tiedemann  observed: 

The  boy  did  not  approve  of  seeing  his  sister  sitting  in  his  chair  of 
wearing  his  clothes;  he  called  these  things  his  things.  [46,  227] 

Here,  the  italicized  pronoun  is  evidence  of  the  widening  ego  for 
mation  with  language  development. 

2  The  records  were  translated  into  French  a  few  years  later  and  into  English 
in  1927. 


A  REVIEW  OF  SOME  STUDIES  ON  EGO  DEVELOPMENT  159 

A  new  impetus  was  given  to  genetic  studies  with  the  theory  of 
evolution  and  with  modern  experimental  psychology.  Charles 
Darwin  [14]  published  A  Biographical  Sketch  of  an  Infant  in 
1877.  His  influence  is  clearly  reflected  a  few  years  later  in  the 
work  of  Preyer,  who,  as  we  shall  see,  actually  repeated  some  of 
Darwin's  methods  in  observing  his  own  child.  At  the  turn  of  the 
century,  genetic  studies  were  made  in  every  country  where  the 
new  evolutionary  theories  spread.8 

Early  in  this  period,  Preyer  observed  the  growth  of  his  son  in 
Germany.  He  wrote: 

Before  the  child  is  in  a  condition  to  recognize  as  belonging  to  him 
the  parts  of  his  body  that  he  can  feel  and  see,  he  must  have  had  a 
great  number  of  experiences,  which  are  for  the  most  part  associated 
with  painful  feelings.  How  little  is  gained  for  the  development  of 
the  notion  of  the  "I"  by  means  of  the  first  movements  of  the  hands, 
which  the  infant  early  carries  to  the  mouth,  and  which  must  give  him, 
when  he  sucks  them,  a  different  feeling  from  that  given  by  sucking 
the  finger  of  another  person,  or  other  suitable  objects,  appears  from 
the  fact  that,  e.g.,  my  child  for  months  tugged  at  his  fingers  as  if  he 
wanted  to  pull  them  off,  and  struck  his  own  head  with  his  hand  by 
way  of  experiment.  At  the  close  of  the  first  year  he  had  a  fancy  for 
striking  hard  substances  against  his  teeth,  and  made  a  regular  play  of 
gnashing  the  teeth.  When  on  the  four  hundred  and  ninth  day  he 
stood  up  straight  in  bed,  holding  on  to  the  railing  of  it  with  his  hands, 
he  bit  himself  on  his  bare  arm,  and  that  the  upper  arm,  so  that  he 
immediately  cried  out  with  pain.  The  marks  of  the  incisors  were  to 
be  seen  long  afterward.  The  child  did  not  a  second  time  bite  himself 
in  the  arm,  but  only  bit  his  fingers,  and  inadvertently  his  tongue. 
[57, 189] 

Thus,  at  a  time  when  the  attention  to  what  is  around  is  already  very 
far  developed,  one's  own  person  may  not  be  distinguished  from  the 
environment.  .  .  .  Nay,  even  in  the  nineteenth  month  it  is  not  yet 
clear  how  much  belongs  to  one's  own  body.  The  child  had  lost  a 
shoe.  I  said,  "Give  the  shoe."  He  stooped,  seized  it,  and  gave  it  to 
me.  Then,  when  I  said  to  the  child,  as  he  was  standing  upright  on 
the  floor,  "Give  the  foot,"  ...  he  grasped  at  it  with  both  hands,  and 
labored  hard  to  get  it  and  hand  it  to  me.  [190] 

*  For  summaries  of  the  work  of  this  period  see  [42]. 


160  THE  GENETIC  FORMATION  OF  THE  EGO 

Another  important  factor  is  the  perception  of  a  change  produced  by 
one's  own  activity  in  all  sorts  of  familiar  objects  that  can  be  taken 
hold  of  in  the  neighborhood;  and  the  most  remarkable  day,  from  a 
psycho-genetic  point  of  view,  in  any  case  an  extremely  significant  day 
in  the  life  of  the  infant,  is  the  one  in  which  he  first  experiences  the 
connection  of  a  movement  executed  by  himself  with  a  sense-impression 
following  upon  it.  [191  j.] 

The  child  that  at  first  merely  played  like  a  cat,  being  amused  with 
color,  form,  and  movement,  has  become  a  causative  being.  Herewith 
the  development  of  the  "I" -feeling  enters  upon  a  new  phase.  [57, 193] 

As  noted  earlier,  Preyer  was  inspired  by  Darwin's  work.    He 
included  the  following  comments  and  quotations  from  Darwin: 

Darwin  recorded  of  one  of  his  sons,  that  in  the  fifth  month  he 
repeatedly  smiled  at  his  father's  image  and  his  own  in  a  mirror  and 
took  them  for  real  objects;  but  he  was  surprised  that  his  father's 
voice  sounded  from  behind  him  (the  child).  "Like  all  infants,  he 
much  enjoyed  thus  looking  at  himself,  and  in  less  than  two  months 
perfectly  understood  that  it  was  an  image,  for  if  I  made  quite  silently 
any  odd  grimace,  he  would  suddenly  turn  around  to  look  at  me. 
.  .  .  The  higher  apes  which  I  tried  with  a  small  looking-glass  behaved 
differently.  They  placed  their  hands  behind  the  glass,  and  in  doing 
so  showed  their  sense;  but,  far  from  taking  pleasure  in  looking  at 
themselves,  they  got  angry  and  would  look  no  more."  [57,  796] 

Preyer  found  that  his  child  also  failed  to  recognize  his  own 
image  at  first: 

In  the  fifty-seventh  week,  however,  I  held  a  small-hand  mirror  close 
to  the  face  of  the  child.  He  looked  at  his  image  and  then  passed  his 
hand  behind  the  glass.  .  .  .  Then  he  took  the  mirror  himself  and 
looked  at  it  and  felt  of  it  on  both  sides.  [57, 198] 

Thus,  Preyer's  child  initially  behaved  similarly  to  Darwin's  apes, 
only  later  learning  that  the  image  was  a  reflection  of  himself. 
Preyer  further  recognized  the  emergence  of  the  ego  at  the  human 
level  as  a  result  of  learning  when  he  wrote: 

More  important  for  the  development  of  the  child's  ego  than  are  the 
observation  of  the  shadow  and  of  the  image  in  the  glass  is  the  learning 
of  speech.  ...  By  means  of  speech  the  conceptual  distinction  of  the 


A  REVIEW  OF  SOME  STUDIES  ON  EGO  DEVELOPMENT  161 

"I,"  the  self,  the  mine,  is  first  made  exact;  the  development,  not  the 
origin,  of  the  "F-feeling  is  simply  favored.  [57,  201  /.] 

A  few  years  after  Preyer's  work  (1895),  J.  M.  Baldwin  [2] 
came  to  similar  conclusions  concerning  ego  formation.  His  obser- 
vations were  casual  and  principally  of  his  own  children.  Without 
subscribing  to  certain  theoretical  extensions  of  Baldwin's  basic 
position  (for  example,  the  recapitulation  theory),  we  include  some 
of  his  generalizations  which  later  investigations  have  substantiated.4 
Baldwin  notes  that 

...  for  a  long  time  the  child's  sense  of  self  includes  too  much.  .  .  , 
It  includes  the  infant's  mother,  and  little  brother,  and  nurse,  in  a  literal 
sense;  ...  To  be  separated  from  his  mother  is  to  lose  a  part  of  him- 
self, as  much  so  as  to  be  separated  from  a  hand  or  foot.  [2,  338  f.] 

However,  through  the  infant's  interaction  with  objects  and  persons 
in  the  environment,  his  "sense  of  self"  becomes  more  clearly 
defined: 

The  ego  and  the  alter  are  thus  born  together.  Both  are  crude  and 
unreflective,  largely  organic,  an  aggregate  of  sensations,  prime  among 
which  are  efforts,  pushes,  strains,  physical  pleasures  and  pains.  .  .  . 
My  sense  of  myself  grows  by  imitation  of  you,  and  my  sense  of  your- 
self grows  in  terms  of  my  sense  of  myself.  Both  ego  and  alter  are  thus 
essentially  social.  [2,  338] 

Baldwin  also  touched  upon  the  problem  of  the  moral  behavior 
of  the  young  child,  later  systematically  developed  by  Piaget.  At 
first,  the  child's  moral  behavior  is  a  copy  of  the  adult's,  followed 
with  some  confusion  and  without  understanding,  but  finally 
learned. 

It  is  its  aim— so  may  the  child  say  to  himself  [of  this  code]  ...  to 
have  me  obey  it,  act  like  it,  think  like  it,  be  like  it  in  all  respects. 
It  is  not  I,  but  I  am  to  become  it.  [2,  345] 

The  fact  that  Baldwin  early  related  the  problem  of  ego  forma- 
tion to  that  of  moral  development  is  especially  significant  as  we 
shall  see  later  on. 

4  The  theoretical  work  of  Baldwin  in  1895  has  been  elaborated  by  C.  H.  Cooley 
[12]  and  George  Mead  [44]  who  were  probably  also  influenced  by  James. 


162  THE  GENETIC  FORMATION  OF  THE  EGO 

Shinn's  studies  [59]  are  among  the  classics  in  the  child  psychol- 
ogy of  this  period.  Beginning  in  1893,  she  carefully  recorded  the 
daily  behavior  of  a  child  for  three  years.  Her  faithful  records 
were  published  in  1899.  More  interested  in  empirical  discovery 
than  in  the  grand  theorizing  of  her  time,  such  as  that  of  James 
and  Baldwin,  Shinn  did  not  publish  a  theoretical  formulation  of 
her  work  until  eight  years  later.  She  conceived  of  ego  develop- 
ment as  a  constant  process  proceeding  from  undiflferentiated 
awareness  of  bodily  movements  to  increasing  differentiation  and 
localization  of  bodily  experiences,  which  emerge  as  "the  large  and 
complex  consciousness  of  the  somatic  self."  [vol.  2,  133]  Shinn 
cites  many  examples  of  experiences  which  were  important  for  the 
separation  of  the  child's  "self"  from  its  external  surroundings. 
For  example,  on  the  132d  day  she  wrote: 

She  was  sitting  in  a  horse  collar  on  the  floor,  and  bent  herself  over 
the  back  of  it  till  the  back  of  her  head  touched  the  floor.  She  righted 
herself  and  tried  again,  with  her  head  turned  sidewise  as  if  to  see  what 
touched  her.  In  this  position,  she  failed  to  reach  the  floor;  she  ... 
tried  again  a  full  dozen  times.  .  .  .  Finally  ...  her  mother  took  her 
up.  She  kept  up  the  same  experiment  for  some  days.  [59,  vol.  1,  143] 

In  a  similar  way,  the  "double  sensations"  involved  in  touching 
parts  of  her  own  body  lead  to  delineation  of  what  is  "me"  and 


"not-mc." 


The  181st  day  her  hand  came  into  contact  with  her  ear;  she  became 
at  once  very  serious,  and  felt  it  and  pulled  it  hard;  losing  it,  she  felt 
around  her  cheek  for  it,  but  when  her  mother  put  her  hand  back,  she 
became  interested  in  the  cheek  and  wished  to  keep  on  feeling  that. 
...  To  the  end  of  the  year,  she  would  .  .  .  feel  over  her  head,  neck, 
hair,  and  ears;  the  hair  she  discovered  in  the  eighth  month,  222d  day, 
while  feeling  for  her  ear,  and  felt  it  over  and  pulled  it  with  great 
curiosity.  [59,  vol.  1, 143] 

The  child  makes  other  "experiments,"  as  Shinn  calls  them,  after 
accidental  contact  with  twigs  and  household  objects,  [vol.  1, 144] 
Still,  a  clear  notion  of  "self"  develops  slowly. 

It  is  possible  that  even  pain  is  late  in  clear  reference  to  the  bodily 
self.  Mrs.  Beatly's  boy,  at  10  months  old,  crying  over  a  bump,  called 


A  REVIEW  OF  SOME  STUDIES  ON  EGO  DEVELOPMENT  163 

attention  to  it  by  pointing  his  finger  to  the  wall  where  he  had  struck 
his  head,  not  at  the  injured  spot.  [vol.  2, 115] 

In  using  sign  language,  in  the  second  year,  babies  always  indicate 
the  mouth,  I  think,  not  the  stomach  region  to  express  hunger.  My 
niece  at  18  months  seems  hardly  to  understand  the  word  "eat"  as 
distinguished  from  putting  into  the  mouth,  and  even  confused  it  with 
"kiss."  [59,  vol.  2, 140] 

However,  a  particularly  severe  pain  was  located  and  remembered. 

In  the  middle  of  the  fourteenth  month,  408th  day,  she  burned  her 
tongue,  seizing  too  hastily  upon  her  potato  .  .  .  three  days  later  when 
conditions  were  repeated,  the  potato  before  her  again  ...  I  asked, 
"Doesn't  Ruth  remember  where  the  potato  burned  her?"  and  she 
nodded  and  put  her  finger  to  her  mouth  to  indicate  the  spot.  As  late 
as  the  446th  day,  hearing  someone  speak  of  burns,  she  put  her  finger 
to  her  mouth  with  a  rueful  sound.  [59,  vol.  1,  151] 

We  see  one  of  the  many  ways  adult  behavior  figured  in  the  early 
development  of  this  child's  ego  when  "in  the  fourteenth  week,  I 
kissed  her  hand  suddenly  and  it  flew  up  as  if  by  reflex,  while  her 
face  showed  surprise."  [vol.  1, 139] 

As  Preyer  noted,  the  development  of  language  greatly  "favors" 
the  process  of  ego  formation.  In  the  following  incident  reported 
by  Shinn,  the  baby's  development  of  a  particular  attitude  toward 
her  own  body  is  indicated.  This  attitude  clearly  depends  upon  the 
segregation  of  the  physical  self  from  the  environment.  The  in- 
fant's use  of  her  own  name  is  particularly  interesting  in  view  of 
later  work  on  the  use  of  personal  pronouns. 

The  first  instance  I  note  of  her  finding  kisses  annoying  is  in  the 
nineteenth  month,  when  twice  she  cried,  "Way!"  (Go  away)  as  I 
was  about  to  kiss  her;  but  during  the  twenty-second  month,  she  often 
showed  annoyance  at  them,  and  I  have  noted  sundry  protests  as, 
"Don't  kiss  Ruth,-hurts!"  [59,  vol.  1,  739] 

Moore  [45]  studied  her  child  at  the  same  time  as  Shinn  with 
the  intent  of  substantiating  the  psychological  theories  of  Wundt. 
She  carefully  noted  the  child's  development  week  by  week.  For 
example: 

Twenty-fourth  wee\:—Ht  did  not  notice,  nor  did  he  appear  to  see, 
a  baby,  but  he  smiled  at  the  woman  who  held  her.  [47] 


164  THE  GENETIC  FORMATION  OF  THE  EGO 

Fiftieth  wee\:—TAt  stood  before  a  mirror  and  made  grimaces  at  his 
own  reflection.  He  stopped  the  performance  upon  perceiving  in  the 
mirror  that  he  was  observed.  [48] 

Fifty-first  weefc—He  observed  another  baby  with  interest.  [50] 
Fifty-eighth  weekj—  ...  He  observed  other  children  closely.  [50] 
Seventy-seventh  wee\:--  ...  he  at  once  recognized  his  own  reflec- 
tion as  that  of  a  "baby."  [45,  62] 

So  this  child,  by  a  variety  of  experience,  not  all  of  which  are 
recorded  or  included  here,  not  only  learned  to  identify  himself, 
but  learned  in  addition  that  others  in  his  world,  occupying  a 
similar  status,  were  somehow  like  himself. 

Moore  also  contributed  observations  which  show  how  the 
development  of  communicable  language  aids  in  the  conceptual 
delineations  of  the  ego. 

As  early  as  the  eighteenth  week,  he  was  able  to  distinguish  between 
mine  and  yours,  and  you  and  /.  It  was  not  until  the  ninety-sixth  week 
that  he  began  to  use  them.  In  the  ninety-seventh  week  he  substituted 
/  for  Warren  and  later  learned  to  speak  of  himself  as  he  probably 
because  he  heard  himself  thus  spoken  of.  [45, 129] 

As  we  shall  see,  this  general  picture  has  been  found  later  by  those 
who  have  investigated  the  development  of  language. 

In  England  during  this  same  period,  James  Sully  [62]  was 
concerned  with  the  development  of  the  "self."  His  theorizing  was 
based  on  a  father's  diary  written  two  years  before  Preyer's,  on 
records  made  by  mothers,  as  well  as  on  autobiographical  material. 
His  conclusions  agree  essentially  with  those  of  Preyer,  Baldwin, 
and  Shinn.  Sully  pointed  out  that  the  child  functions  as  an 
organism  before  he  has  any  psychological  experience  of  "self,"  a 
fact  emphasized  much  later  by  Piaget  and  Susan  Isaacs: 

The  most  distant  acquaintance  with  the  first  years  of  human  life 
tells  us  that  young  children  have  much  in  common  with  the  lower 
animals.  Their  characteristic  passions  and  impulses  are  centred  in  self 
and  the  satisfaction  of  its  wants.  [62,  231] 

Resistances  from  physical  objects  help  the  child  experience  "this 
consciousness  of  self  in  its  antagonism  to  a  not-self."  [235] 


A  REVIEW  OF  SOME  STUDIES  ON  EGO  DEVELOPMENT  165 

The  child's  entrance  into  social  life  through  a  growing  consciousness 
of  the  existence  of  others  is  marked  by  much  fierce  opposition  to  their 
wishes.  [231] 

The  acquisition  of  clearer  ideas  about  self  and  others  has  been 
touched  on  in  connexion  with  the  growth  of  the  boy's  language.  The 
first  use  of  "I"  and  the  contemporaneous  first  use  of  "you"  (end  of 
third  month)  [of  second  year  of  life]  seem  to  point  to  a  new  awaken- 
ing of  the  intelligence  to  the  mystery  of  self,  and  of  its  unique  position 
in  relation  to  other  things.  [444] 

By  the  end  of  the  fourth  month  [of  the  second  year]  we  read  that 
"I"  was  growing  less  shy,  not  merely  coming  on  the  scene  in  familiar 
and  safe  verbal  companionship,  as  in  expressions  like  "I  can,"  but 
boldly  pushing  its  way  alone  or  in  new  combinations.  By  the  sixth 
month  (two  and  a  half)  the  name  Ningi  may  be  said  to  have  dis- 
appeared from  his  [child's]  vocabulary.  His  rejection  of  it  was  for- 
mally announced  at  the  age  of  two  years  seven  and  a  half  months. 
On  being  asked  at  this  date  whether  he  was  Ningi  he  answered, 
"No,  my  name  Kiffie  [Cliffie]."  He  then  added,  "Ningi  name  of 
another  little  boy."  [62,  445] 

All  these  observations  are  important  for  our  theme.  We  see  that 
the  child's  behavior  is  first  governed  by  biological  needs.  By  en- 
countering resistances  of  various  sorts,  this  "consciousness  of  self, 
in  its  antagonism  to  a  not-self"  begins  to  form.  During  this  period, 
the  "corporeal  reference"  of  self  is  clearly  manifested.  [426]  But 
with  the  growth  of  language,  the  ego  formation  becomes  at  the 
same  time  more  precise  and  complex,  for  the  conceptualization  of 
"self"  thus  afforded  is  accompanied  by  notions  (attitudes)  of  self 
in  relation  to  others,  as  we  saw  in  Sully's  illustrations  when  Cliffie 
renounced  his  baby  name. 

Still  during  this  elaboration  of  the  ego,  confusion  as  to  the 
limitations  of  "I"  does  occur,  particularly  when  the  child  encoun- 
ters new  experiences.  Sully  reports  from  George  Sand's  account 
of  her  childhood  an  incident  that  occurred  when  she  was  four 
years  old: 

It  was  at  Madrid  that  she  first  made  acquaintance  with  one  of 
Nature's  most  fascinating  mysteries,  the  echo. 

"I  studied  this  phenomenon  with  an  extreme  pleasure.  What  struck 
me  as  most  strange  was  to  hear  my  own  name  repeated  by  my  own 
voice.  Then  there  occurred  to  me  an  odd  explanation.  I  thought 


166  THE  GENETIC  FORMATION  OF  THE  EGO 

that  I  'was  double,  and  that  there  was  round  about  me  another  T 
whom  I  could  not  sec,  but  who  always  saw  me,  since  he  always  an- 
swered me." 

She  spent  days  in  trying  to  get  sight  of  her  double.  Her  mother 
.  .  .  told  her  it  was  echo,  "the  voice  in  the  air!"  [62,  496] 

The  observations  relevant  to  the  development  of  the  ego  were 
sufficiently  well-known  and  in  agreement  to  be  summarized  in 
1909  by  Tracy  and  Stimpfl  [63]  in  their  text.  In  a  section  entitled 
"The  Idea  of  Self,"  the  authors  speak  of 

The  phenomena  which  accompany  and  indicate  the  gradual  emer- 
gence into  clear  consciousness,  of  what  Taine  calls  the  "unextended 
centre,"  the  "mathematical  point,"  by  relation  to  which  all  the  "other" 
is  defined  and  which  each  of  us  calls  "I,"  or  "me."  [63,  70] 

As  we  have  pointed  out  in  chapter  5,  ego  components  do  indeed 
function  as  reference  points  or  anchorages  in  terms  of  which  the 
individual  relates  himself  to  his  immediate  environment  and  to  the 
social  milieu. 

It  should  be  mentioned  in  passing  that  some  sociologists  of  the 
early  1900's  were  also  concerned  with  ego  genesis.  In  1908,  Cooley 
[13]  studied  the  use  of  "self  words"  by  a  child  to  see  "how  far 
and  in  what  sense  the  self-idea  is  a  social  conception."  [339]  He 
found  the  use  of  personal  pronouns,  as  of  all  communicable  lan- 
guage, inextricably  bound  with  social  interaction.  He  concluded: 

"7"  is  a  differentiation  in  a  vague  body  of  personal  ideas1  which  is 
either  self-consciousness  or  social  consciousness,  as  you  please  to  loof( 
at  it.  [13,  342 f  italics  ours] 

William  Stern  [60]  referred  to  the  process  of  ego  formation 
when  he  wrote: 

At  first,  however,  there  is  no  ego-consciousness,  no  objective  con- 
sciousness, but  only  the  very  first  germs,  entirely  undifferentiated  of 
both.  [60,  76] 

These  "very  first  germs"  Stern  calls  "sense  emotional  states."  [75] 
It  is  not  our  purpose  here  to  speculate  upon  the  "consciousness"  of 
an  infant.  However,  it  is  significant  that  Stern  too  concluded 
from  his  own  study  and  those  of  early  investigators  (for  example, 
Preyer  and  Shinn)  that  differentiation  between  the  ego  and  the 


A  REVIEW  OF  SOME  STUDIES  ON  EGO  DEVELOPMENT  167 

outside  world  ("objective  consciousness")  occurs  in  the  course  of 
development  and  is  not  present  at  birth.  Stern  also  wrote  that  the 
child's 

.  . .  life-circle,  of  which  his  ego  is  the  very  centre,  extends  at  first 
only  by  slow  degrees;  at  first  he  has  to  get  a  sure  and  firm  footing 
before  he  is  capable  of  entering  into  living  relations  with  his  environ- 
ment and  the  strange  aims  it  presents  to  him.  Hence  the  desires  and 
impulses  that  first  develop  are  above  all  of  an  egoistic  nature.  [60, 
493] 

Stern's  material  concerning  play  and  fantasy  in  early  childhood 
is  also  revealing: 

When  we  see  the  child's  absolute  absorption  as  he  listens  to  a  fairy- 
tale and  tells  an  imaginary  tale  of  his  own,  how  earnestly  he  carries  on 
his  games,  and  his  despair  at  any  interruption,  then  we  recognize  that 
the  illusion  of  reality  is  here  complete  indeed,  or  very  nearly  so.  ... 
And  yet  the  child  begins  fairly  early  to  feel  dimly  that  other  stern 
idea  of  reality  possessed  by  us  adults.  He  notices  that  many  of  his 
conscious  experiences  cannot  be  ended  or  changed  in  accordance  with 
his  fancy,  but  that  they  force  themselves  upon  him  and  make  their 
consequences  felt;  in  short,  he  begins  to  realize  his  dependence  on 
things  outside  himself  and  to  endeavour  to  adapt  himself  to  them. 
[60,  283  f.f  italics  ours] 

The  classic  work  of  Piaget  and  his  collaborators  gives  invaluable 
information  on  the  development  of  the  ego.  In  fact,  Piaget 
[52-56]  systematically  links  the  major  psychological  functions 
of  infant  mentality  with  the  initial  lack  of  and  the  progressive 
development  of  the  ego.  On  the  basis  of  actual  material  collected 
over  a  period  of  years,  Piaget  concludes  that  "the  younger  the 
child,  the  less  sense  he  has  of  his  own  ego."  [55,  86]  In  early  in- 
fancy, "the  child  does  not  distinguish  between  external  and  inter- 
nal, subjective  and  objective."  [86]  * 

The  infant  psychologically  first  floats  "about  in  an  undiflferen- 
tiated  absolute."  [54, 128]  In  this  undiflferentiated  absolute  there 
are  no  psychological  boundaries  between  one's  own  body  and  other 
objects,  between  reality  and  phantasy.  Thus  a  distinct  ego  experi- 

5  See  also  [53,  197}  and  [54,  130]. 


168  THE  GENETIC  FORMATION  OF  THE  EGO 

cnce  is  the  "result  of  a  gradual  and  progressive  dissociation,  and 
not  a  primitive  intuition."  [128]  The  dominant  principle  regulat- 
ing the  orientation  of  this  initial  "undifferentiated  absolute"  is  the 
satisfaction  of  the  momentary  needs  or  wishes  as  they  arise.  Be- 
cause child  mentality  and  behavior  are  governed  by  the  "pleasure 
principle,"  to  use  the  term  Piaget  borrowed  from  psychoanalysis, 
the  child  reacts  differently  to  the  same  objects  when  his  needs  or 
wishes  change.  The  infant  may  react  positively  toward  a  person 
or  object  at  one  instant,  negatively  a  little  later.  He  has  as  yet 
established  no  consistent  role  and  status  relationships  with  other 
individuals  because  consistent  ego  links  are  lacking.  From  the 
point  of  view  of  adult  logic,  such  behavior  is  characterized  by 
contradictions.  However,  this  so-called  inconsistency  is  consistent 
in  that  it  follows  (or  is  regulated  by)  the  variations  in  needs  or 
wishes  as  they  momentarily  arise.  This  is  the  stage  of  pure  autism, 
and  autism  "knows  of  no  adaptation  to  reality,  because  pleasure  is 
its  only  spring  of  action."  [53,  244]  As  we  shall  see  later,  this 
undifferentiated  state  is  important  in  understanding  the  systematic 
extensions  of  Piaget's  theory  to  ethical  and  social  fields. 

Because  of  the  resistances  met  in  the  external  world,  the  child 
adapts  to  reality.  But  in  order  to  adapt,  he  must  begin  to  make 
distinctions  between  what  is  himself  and  what  is  not.  In  this 
process,  logical  consistency  dawns  and  gradually  develops  through 
the  stages  of  "egocentrism"  and  logical  thinking.  In  this  "ego- 
centric" period,  the  child  talks  and  acts  as  if  he  were  the  center  of 
reference  of  the  whole  world.  There  is  not  much  logical  consist- 
ency, for  this  can  be  achieved  only  when  some  well-established 
premise  is  followed  step  by  step.  In  the  process  of  ego  develop- 
ment, differentiation  takes  place.  Realization  of  the  reciprocal 
relations  between  oneself  and  other  people  evolves  as  one  learns 
that  there  are  other  points  of  view  besides  one's  own  absolutism. 
In  order  to  grasp  this,  the  child  must  be  able  to  separate  himself 
from  the  external  world. 

Although  the  age  locations  of  these  stages,  the  situations  in 
which  the  highest  and  lowest  coefficients  of  "egocentrism"  may  be 
obtained,  the  factors  influencing  the  duration  or  overlapping  of 
"egocentric"  and  "logical"  stages  are  important  problems  for  child 
and  developmental  psychology,  they  are  not  our  present  concern. 


A  REVIEW  OF  SOME  STUDIES  ON  EGO  DEVELOPMENT  169 

As  we  shall  see,  social,  material  (economic),  and  physical  condi- 
tions in  different  social  systems  and  classes  in  which  the  child 
grows  may  accelerate  or  retard  the  appearance  of  various  stages. 
Even  the  particular  peculiarities,  economic  and  personal,  of  a  spe- 
cific family  might  produce  differential  timing.  The  value  of 
Piaget's  work  for  us  lies  chiefly  in  the  demonstration  of  the  general 
trend  of  a  child's  developing  mentality,  starting  from  an  undiffer- 
entiated  absolute  dominated  by  autism,  governed  chiefly  by  the 
satisfaction  of  momentary  needs  or  wishes,  through  the  gradual 
adaptation  to  reality  as  the  child  meets  external  resistances.  Dur- 
ing this  process  the  ego  develops. 

In  the  impressive  work  of  Henri  Wallon,  Les  Origines  du  carae- 
tire  chez  I* enfant  [64],  which  consists  chiefly  of  lectures  delivered 
at  the  Sorbonne  from  1929  to  1931,  we  find  descriptions  of  the 
affective  reactions  of  the  newborn  child  as  well  as  a  detailed  ac- 
count of  the  progressive  development  of  the  experience  and  differ- 
entiation of  one's  own  body  [155-210]  and  a  consciousness  of  the 
ego.  [210-265]  This  latter  section  of  Wallon's  work  is  particularly 
important  for  our  present  problem.  For  he  brings  together  here 
the  diverse  factors  in  genetic  development  which  contribute  to  the 
formation  of  the  ego.  Starting  with  the  conception  that  the  ego 
is  not  innately  given  as  a  unitary  experience  at  birth,  he  shows  step 
by  step  how  the  experience  of  the  self  is  achieved  as  something 
distinct  from  the  surrounding  objects  and  persons  as  the  child 
encounters  them  in  the  course  of  his  development.  This  distinc- 
tion is  achieved  only  after  a  progressive  and  laborious  process  of 
assimilation  and  identification.6 

Moral  character  appears  only  as  a  consequence  of  the  child's 
differentiation  of  himself  from  other  individuals  and  his  aware- 

6  "La  conscience  de  soi  n'est  pas  essentielle  et  primitive,  comme  le  postulent 
ceux  qui  en  font  I'instrument  de  la  psychologic.  Elle  est  un  produit  deja  tres 
differencie  de  Pactivite  psychique.  C'est  seulement  a  partir  de  trois  ans  que  Penfant 
commence  a  se  conduire  et  a  se  connaitre  en  sujet  distinct  d'autrui.  Et  pour  qu'il 
arrive  a  s'analyser,  a  chercher  les  formules  a  Paide  desquelles  il  tentera  d'exprimer 
son  individualite  subjective,  il  lui  faut  subir  une  evolution  qui  le  mene  jusqu'a 
Fadolescence  ou  a  Page  adulte  et  dont  les  degres  et  les  formes  varient  conside- 
rablement  d'une  personne  a  Pautre."  [209]  "Petit  a  petit  cependant,  par  un  long 
travail  d'identification  et  d'assimilation,  il  apprend  simultanement  a  dechiffrer, 
dans  ses  impressions,  le  monde  qui  s'oppose  a  lui  et  a  s'attribuer  comme  sien  ce 
qui  va  le  rendre  capable  d'opposer  a  autrui  les  exigences  de  sa  personne.*'  [210] 


170  THE  GENETIC  FORMATION  OF  THE  EGO 

ness  of  the  reciprocal  relationship  he  has  with  others.  [205, 221  ff.] 
Wallon  notes  clearly  that  the  notions  and  values  attached  to  the 
developing  ego  and  personality  will  vary  according  to  the  particu- 
lar material  and  social  circumstances  that  surround  the  particular 
child  in  question.7 

Susan  Isaacs  [31,  32],  with  her  psychoanalytic  approach,  places 
less  emphasis  on  the  social  factors  in  development  and  criticizes 
Piaget's  systematic  presentation  on  several  counts.  Without  enter- 
ing into  controversies,  we  see  that  Isaacs'  results  nevertheless  sub- 
stantiate the  general  conclusions  reached  by  other  investigators 
reviewed,  including  Piaget.  Her  work  again  shows  that  the  ego 
is  not  a  primitive  perception  but  appears  in  the  course  of  contacts 
with  the  external  world.  She  remarks  that 

The  first  value  which  the  physical  world  has  for  the  child  is  as  a 
canvas  upon  which  to  project  his  personal  wishes  and  anxieties,  and 
his  first  form  of  interest  in  it  is  one  of  dramatic  representation.  [31, 
101] 

To  him,  his  wishes,  desires  or  urges,  call  them  what  we  will,  together 
with  their  outcome  in  emotions,  are  the  one  reality.  [32,  286] 

There  is  as  yet  no  differentiated  ego.  Isaacs  gives  a  vivid  picture 
of  early  child  mentality  in  which  the  ego  is  not  yet  formed: 

In  the  same  way,  things  "inside"  himself  literally  mean  to  the  child 
ir\^ide  Ms  body.  And  when  the  child  takes  the  parents  as  a  controlling 
agent,  it  seems  to  him  that  they  are  thereafter  inside  his  actual  body. 
They  become  identified  with  internal  body  processes  such  as  intes- 
tinal movements,  stomach  pains,  breathing,  arid  so  on;  and  even  with 
actual  body  substances,  for  example,  faeces  and  urine.  [32,  295] 

With  frustrations  of  the  child's  instincts  (resistances  from  his  en- 
vironment, including  that  of  parents  and  other  persons)  starts  the 
"appreciation  of  the  external  world."  [288] 

7  "En  particulier,  dans  la  periode  qui  suit,  clle  va  s'intcgrer  plus  ou  moms 
e'troitement  a  la  notion  qui  developpera  chez  Penfant  la  conscience  de  sa  per- 
sonnalite  morale  vis-a-vis  d'autrui.  Elle  ne  peut  manquer  aussi  d'etre  plus  ou 
moins  a  Pimage  des  representations  suivant  lesquelles  il  apprend  dc  Padulte  a 
clefinir  scs  rapports  avcc  le  milieu  physique  ct  social.  Et  clle  se  trouve  ainsi 
modelee  par  les  conditions  dc  vie  ct  dc  pcnsec  ou  le  placent  les  techniques  dc 
rcxistence,  les  formes  du  language,  les  usages,  croyances,  connaissances,  etc., 
propre  a  son  epoque.  Ces  variations  ne  semblent  pas  avoir  d'autre  limite  quc  la 
diversite  possible  des  civilisations."  [205  f.] 


A  REVIEW  OF  SOME  STUDIES  ON  EGO  DEVELOPMENT  171 

Lewin  [39,  40]  portrays  the  developing  ego  in  line  with  the 
Gestalt  understanding  of  the  problem.8  The  ego  is  referred  to  as 
a  "system  or  complex  of  systems,  a  functional  part  region  within 
(the)  psychological  totality."  [40,  56]  The  development  of  the 
ego  is  traced  from  the  "undifferentiated  absolute."  When  the 
child  meets  the  resistances  of  inanimate  objects,  and  the  opposi- 
tions offered  by  other  people,  such  as  parents  or  nurses,  he  has  to 
distinguish  between  himself  and  external  things.  [175] 

Analogous  to  this  relatively  slight  delimitation  among  the  various 
inner  psychological  systems,  the  functional  firmness  of  the  boundary 
between  his  own  person  and  the  psychological  environment  is  also 
in  general  less  with  the  child  than  with  the  adult.  This  is  expressed, 
for  example,  by  the  fact,  that  "I"  or  self  is  only  gradually  formed, 
perhaps  in  the  second  or  third  year.  [39, 121] 

And  the  slighter  "firmness  of  the  boundary"  between  self  and  en- 
vironment has  a  direct  bearing  upon  the  slighter  separation  of  real 
from  unreal  strata. 

As  we  have  seen  and  shall  consider  in  more  detail  later,  segrega- 
tion of  the  ego  from  the  persons  and  objects  in  the  child's  environ- 
ment, language  development,  and  the  formation  of  attitudes  to- 
wards himself  and  others  are  necessary  for  the  child's  grasp  of 
reciprocal  relationships.  As  Charlotte  Biihler  [7,  8]  wrote: 

The  child  is  a  part  of  his  mother  before  he  becomes  an  individual 
for  himself  and  is  part  of  a  definite  group  for  a  long  time  before  he 
can  enter  and  join  any  group  actively.  [7,  380] 

Although  some  of  Buhler's  observations  would  seem  to  throw  light 
on  the  problem  of  ego  formation,  she  refers  to  it  only  in  passing: 

Unfortunately,  we  know  as  yet  very  little  about  the  entire  genesis 
of  the  sense  of  ego  in  the  child.  [8,  74] 

Horowitz'  study  [27]  on  "localization"  of  the  ego  is  very  much 
to  the  point  in  our  review  of  investigations  indicating  that  the  ego 
is  a  genetic  formation.  The  problem  in  this  study  was  to  find  at 

8  Sec  Kohler's  Gestalt  Psychology  [38]  and  Koffka's  Principles  of  Gestalt  Psy- 
chology [37]. 


172  THE  GENETIC  FORMATION  OF  THE  EGO 

what  point  in  the  body  each  individual  subject  located  the  "self." 
Horowitz  varied  the  procedure  to  suit  the  ages  of  the  subjects, 
small  children  and  college  students.  The  results  show  that  differ- 
ent subjects  located  themselves  in  different  parts  of  the  body,  for 
example,  head,  face,  brain,  heart,  genitals,  chest.  Horowitz  con- 
cluded from  the  results  that 

The  localization  of  the  self  as  is  reported  in  the  literature  quoted, 
in  the  responses  on  our  questionnaire,  in  informal  discussion,  in  the 
investigation  of  children,  is  not  the  basic  phenomenon  one  might  hope 
for  to  ease  an  analysis  of  the  structure  of  the  self  and  personality. 
The  more  or  less  stable  and  constant  association  of  the  self-concept 
with  the  particular  body  regions,  functions,  or  external  objects  or 
conditions  serves  chiefly  as  a  reference  point  for  the  individual  as  a 
whole  in  the  situation.  [27,  386] 

No  adequate  survey  of  representative  observations  relevant  to 
the  problem  of  ego  formation  could  be  written  without  use  of  the 
vast  data  accumulated  over  many  years  by  Gesell  and  his  asso- 
ciates. [19-22]  In  their  1943  publication,  Gesell  and  Ilg  sum- 
marize these  observations  as  "behavior  norms."  However,  as  early 
as  1934,  Gesell  and  Thompson  included  observations  of  ego  forma- 
tion. For  example,  the  babies'  behavior  before  a  mirror  was  noted: 

At  40  weeks  ...  the  responses  tend  to  be  somewhat  delayed  and 
even  restrained  and  sober.  [21,  242] 

At  56  weeks,  on  the  contrary,  social,  outgoing  response  to  the  image 
is  characteristically  prompt  and  prominent.  The  infant  seems  to  be 
completely  deceived  and  he  acts  as  though  he  were  in  full  social 
commerce  with  another  child.  [241] 

It  is  doubtful  whether  the  infant  identifies  himself  in  any  way  with 
the  image.  Even  at  the  age  of  5  years  a  pair  of  twin  girls  made 
misinterpretations  of  their  mirror  images.  Each  considered  the  image 
not  a  self-image,  but  called  it  by  the  name  of  the  cotwin.  [21,  241] 

In  the  latter  example,  the  peculiar  situation  of  having  a  live  "self- 
image"  caused  confusion  in  one  aspect  of  delineating  the  ego  from 
what  was  outside  the  ego.  In  1938,  "mirror  behavior"  was  included 
among  other  "behavior  norms." 

In  1940,  the  description  of  the  first  five  years  of  life  is  rich  with 
observations  such  as  these: 


A  REVIEW  OF  SOME  STUDIES  ON  EGO  DEVELOPMENT  173 

The  1-year-old  infant  is  not  very  articulate.  .  .  .  Much  of  his  emo- 
tional expression  is  highly  egocentric.  He  makes  very  meager  dis- 
tinction between  himself  and  others.  His  vocalizations  are  only 
beginning  to  have  a  social  reference.  [19,  31  /.] 

[At  eighteen  months:]  His  social  insights  are  not  much  more  bril- 
liant than  his  perception  of  eliminative  functions.  He  is  self -en  grossed 
(not  selfish)  because  he  does  not  perceive  other  persons  as  individuals 
life  himself.  \33,  italics  ours] 

[At  two  years  of  age:]  Pronouns,  mine,  me,  you,  and  /  are  coming 
into  use  approximately  in  the  order  just  given.  While  his  sense  of 
self  is  not  as  totalitarian  as  it  was  at  eighteen  months,  it  is  by  no  means 
sufficiently  defined  for  conceptual  verbalization.  He  is  much  more 
prone  to  call  himself  by  his  given  name:  "Peter  slide  down,"  instead 
of  "I  slide  down."  [37] 

You  can  bargain  with  Three.  He  knows  with  a  clarity  that  was 
quite  wanting  at  Two  that  he  is  a  person  and  that  you  are  a  person, 
and  he  negotiates  reciprocal  trade  agreements.  [44] 

He  must  have  a  sense  of  self  and  of  status  because  he  somewhat 
disdains  such  a  simple,  babyish  commission  as,  "Show  me,  where  is 
your  nose!"  [44] 

[The  four-year-old]  is  even  beginning  to  sense  himself  as  one  among 
many.  He  is  less  circumscribed  than  Three.  He  has  a  definite 
consciousness  of  kind,  of  his  own  kind.  Once  during  a  psychological 
examination  he  asked,  "Do  you  spank  children  who  don't  finish?" 
A  revealing  question,  which  discloses  that  the  4-year-old  realizes  his 
equivalence  with  other  children  who  come  to  the  Clinic  under  similar 
circumstances.  [19, 48] 

These  generalizations  based  on  observations  conducted  under 
conditions  similar  to  strict  laboratory  controls  elaborate  the  general 
picture  given  by  earlier  investigators.  With  the  realization  that 
particular  behavior  at  particular  times  is  affected  in  a  major  way 
by  the  circumstances  of  development  and  learning,  both  large  and 
small,  we  can  review  the  summary  of  Gesell  and  Ilg  [20]  on  ego 
development  "in  the  culture  of  today." 

The  newborn  infant  has  no  "clear  sense  of  self-identity." 

But  as  he  grows  up  he  must  disengage  himself  from  this  univer- 
sality, and  become  a  well-defined  individual.  By  the  time  he  is  five 
or  six  years  old  he  must  see  himself  for  what  he  is.  [20,  334] 


174  THE  GENETIC  FORMATION  OF  THE  EGO 

As  he  grows,  he  must  separate  himself  from  his  environment  by 
"experience  which  sometimes  is  bitter."  Step  by  step,  he  under- 
goes 

...  a  progressive  differentiation  which  disengages  him  more  fully 
from  the  culture  in  which  he  is  so  deeply  involved.  Paradoxically  this 
very  disengagement  also  identifies  him  more  fully  with  his  culture; 
he  transforms  from  a  mere  ward  to  a  working  member.  [ 334  /.] 

At  4  weekjs  of  age,  the  baby's  face  is  generally  impassive.  By 
8  wee\s  it  breaks  into  a  spontaneous  social  smile  at  the  sight  of 
another  person's  face  .  .  .  this  double  reference  is  at  the  basis  of  the 
child's  socialization  and  personalization.  At  16  weefy  the  social  smile 
is  spontaneous  or  self-induced.  ...  By  25  wee\s  he  already  reacts 
differently  to  a  stranger's  face.  [335] 

By  40  wee\s  or  a  year  he  has  made  significant  advances  in  self- 
discovery.  Whether  sitting  up  or  lying  down,  his  arms  and  hands 
now  have  more  freedom  of  movement,  and  he  uses  them  to  explore 
his  own  physical  self.  At  16  weeks  they  were  at  his  mouth;  at  20 
weeks  they  engaged  above  his  chest.  Following  the  head  to  foot  trend 
which  is  characteristic  of  development,  the  hands  at  40  weeks  (and 
later)  come  down  to  the  thighs.  Just  as  he  used  to  indulge  in  mutual 
fingering,  he  now  makes  contact  with  his  genitals  when  not  clothed. 
(336} 

In  the  period  from  1  to  2  years  there  is  an  increasing  amount  of 
social  reference.  Although  the  infant-child  is  capable  of  long  stretches 
of  self-absorbed  activity,  he  is  also  given  to  numerous  social  advances. 
...  He  extends  a  toy  to  a  person;  he  holds  out  his  arm  for  the  sleeve; 
he  says  "ta-ta";  he  hands  the  empty  cereal  dish  to  his  mother;  .  .  . 
By  all  these  tokens  and  devices,  he  builds  up  a  vast  body  of  specific 
perceptual  experience  which  ultimately  enables  him  to  draw  the 
momentous  conclusion  that  there  are  other  persons  in  the  world  more 
or  less  like  himself.  [336] 

Two  years  is  a  transitional  period  when  the  child  both  clings  to 
moorings  and  cuts  from  them.  Johnny  is  his  name,  and  in  his  in- 
articulate psychology,  the  spoken  word  Johnny  which  he  hears  is 
nothing  more  or  less  than  he  himself!  His  name  is  Johnny  as  a 
person.  He  will  soon  use  the  pronouns  you,  me,  and  /, — a  further 
indication  of  a  fundamental  change  in  the  psychology  of  his  self. 
[337,  italics  ours] 

"Even  by  the  age  of  3  years,  the  child  has  attained  a  well-balanced 
sense  of  self.  ...  He  knows  his  own  sex  with  assurance.  His  inter- 


A  REVIEW  OF  SOME  STUDIES  ON  EGO  DEVELOPMENT  175 

est  in  human  anatomy  remains  strong;  he  talks  freely  and  naturally 
about  differences  which  he  has  observed.  [20,  338  /.] 

At  four  years,  Gesell  notes:  "On  occasion,  with  bravado,  he 
would  like  to  be  a  man  of  the  world;  but  he  really  is  still  closely 
bound  to  his  mother.  He  cites  her  as  authority  in  cases  of  dispute. 
MY  mommie  s^ys  so!"  [339]  Here,  and  in  the  following  quota- 
tion from  Gesell,  we  see  how  the  child  reifies  the  dictums  of 
grownups: 

Between  5  and  6  years  the  child  ...  has  lost  some  of  the  sopho- 
moric  traits  of  4  year  oldness,  and  has  more  sense  o£  status  and  pro- 
priety. He  has  a  better  appreciation  of  the  folkways  of  culture.  He 
shows  the  conservatism  of  youth  in  deferring  to  them,  and  citing  them 
to  his  parents  for  their  consideration.  He  does  not  want  to  be 
different  from  humanity.  [20,  340] 

Another  observation,  valuable  for  our  discussion,  is,  that  at  about 
72  months  the  child  begins  "setting  up  standards  for  himself," 
forming  value  judgments  about  his  own  behavior. 

We  have  not  tried  in  this  review  to  discover  all  the  details  of  the 
progressive  development  of  the  ego.  The  variety  of  experiences, 
objects,  and  persons  important  for  different  children  in  different 
situations  is  impressive.  From  the  observations  of  both  early  and 
more  recent  investigators,  it  has  become  evident,  however,  that  the 
ego  is  not  given  at  birth  but  is  a  genetic  formation.  The  infant  at 
first  makes  no  differentiation  between  his  body  and  its  surround- 
ings. When  resistances  (especially  unpleasant  ones)  are  met  from 
the  surroundings  (objects  and  persons),  the  gradual  process  of 
segregating  "self"  from  "not  self"  is  furthered.  As  the  child  ma- 
tures and  as  communicable  language  is  learned,  the  ego  becomes 
more  precisely  defined  and  is  extended  by  the  child  with  his  own 
conceptions  (attitudes)  about  the  body  and  other  objects,  persons, 
and  situations.  This  general  trend  has  been  repeatedly  observed 
even  though  the  process  of  ego  formation  does  show  variable 
aspects  when  different  children  are  viewed  under  different  condi- 
tions. As  we  noted  previously,  these  facts  and  their  important 
implications  for  the  socialization  of  the  child  have  not  been  system- 
atically brought  out  in  general  books  on  child  psychology.  How- 
ever, the  facts  have  contributed  to  the  formulations  of  some 


176  THE  GENETIC  FORMATION  OF  THE  EGO 

developmental  psychologists  and  are  almost  taken  for  granted. 
For  example,  in  her  recent  book  (1945),  Goodenough  notes  that 
goals  and  values  can  be  established  and  plans  for  the  future  can  be 
made  only  after  a  certain  development  of  the  ego.  [24] 

Nothing  is  more  important  for  the  child's  future  than  the  level  and 
type  of  the  goals  he  sets  for  himself.  And  because  these  goals  are  not 
clearly  realized  until  after  the  crystallizing  effect  of  verbal  formulation 
has  taken  place  and  the  distinction  between  the  self  and  the  not-self 
has  become  sufficiently  advanced  to  give  form  and  pattern  to  the 
child's  social  attitudes,  the  period  of  middle  childhood  takes  on  special 
importance,  for  it  is  then  that  the  child  for  the  first  time  faces  his 
future.  [24,  423,  italics  ours] 

Such  considerations  should  be  paramount  in  dealing  with  the 
problems  of  attitude  formation  in  general  and  ego  differentiation 
in  particular. 

Language  development  and  ego  formation.  Several  investiga- 
tors (for  example,  Preyer,  Moore,  Cooley  and  Gesell)  have  stressed 
the  fact  that  ego  formation  is  accelerated  and  expanded  as  the 
child  acquires  a  more  effective  use  of  language.  Since  language 
development  looms  so  important  in  the  genetic  formation  of  the 
ego,  at  least  a  few  paragraphs  emphasizing  its  role  are  necessary. 

A  number  of  investigations  of  language  development  have 
accumulated  in  recent  years.  We  can  refer  here  to  only  a  few  of 
them  to  show  what  valuable  information  they  furnish  concerning 
the  conceptualization  of  the  self.  Since,  as  we  shall  see  later,  the 
results  of  most  child  study  are,  to  a  greater  or  lesser  degree,  de- 
pendent upon  the  particular  situations  under  which  observation  is 
made,  it  is  not  our  problem  here  to  consider  the  exact  percentages 
of  any  language  behavior  in  question  found  at  different  age  levels 
and  under  different  conditions.  The  relevance  of  all  these  results 
to  the  present  discussion  is  the  consistent  trend  reported. 

McCarthy  [43]  observed  children  when  they  were  in  familiar 
situations.  Her  analysis  revealed  that  pronouns  "represent  about 
10  per  cent  of  the  total  number  of  words  used  by  the  eighteen- 
month-old  children,  and  they  increase  to  approximately  20  per  cent 
of  the  words  used  by  the  fifty-four-month-old  children."  [725] 
Pronouns,  of  course,  serve  as  conceptualizations  of  the  self  and 


A  REVIEW  OF  SOME  STUDIES  ON  EGO  DEVELOPMENT  177 

others.  Fisher's  observations  [17]  of  children  from  18  to  60  months 
of  age  in  nursery  school  situations  also  show  a  progressive  increase 
in  the  use  of  pronouns  in  relation  to  one's  self  and  others  with  age. 
She  found  "a  significant  positive  relationship  between  use  of  we, 
our,  and  us  and  chronological  age."  [74]  No  child  used  these 
pronouns  before  the  first  half  of  the  third  year.  In  Fisher's  words, 
this  increase  with  age  suggests  "a  gradually  increasing  awareness 
of  membership  in  a  larger  group."  [56] 

The  almost  traditional  interest  of  some  sociologists  in  the  process 
of  ego  formation  is  shown  again  by  Bain's  observations  on  his 
child.  [1]  Noting  that  his  study  agreed  with  those  of  Preyer, 
Shinn,  and  Cooley,  he  wrote  "that  infants  learn  the  names  of 
others  before  they  learn  their  own."  The  following  excerpts  from 
his  records  give  some  idea  of  the  development  of  the  conceptual 
"I"  in  Bain's  child.  Referring  to  herself  first  as  "baby,"  then  as 
"See"  (Sheila),  the  child  used  "I"  only  after  some  confusion. 

Months 
and  Days 

14-8.  S  distinguishes  "daddy's  ear"  from  "your  car." 

16-0.  When  told  to  say  "Sheila  Bain"  she  says  "Baby!" 

16-12.  S  says  "baby"  when  told  to  say  her  own  name  and  also  whenever 

she  sees  another  baby,  doll,  child,  or  picture  of  any  of  them. 

17-18.  Posscssives  arc  quite  well  established:  "See's  Dadda,  See's  Mama" 

20-17.        Used  "my"  for  "I"— -"My  see  you." 

26-18.        Tonight  S  said  "I  want  to!    I  want  to!"    R  [her  mother]  said, 
"Who  is  I?"    S  said  "I  is  me!"    [1] 

Thus  we  find,  four  decades  later,  substantiation  of  Mdore's  obser- 
vation that  the  child  refers  to  himself  with  his  own  name  before 
he  uses  "I"  or  "me,"  a  fact  by  now  familiar  to  all  students  of  child 
psychology.  When  the  "I"  emerges  clearly,  a  step  forward  is  taken 
in  the  complex  process  of  forming  attitudes  toward  the  self  and 
others. 

Using  the  careful  techniques  of  modern  child  study,  Good- 
enough  [23]  observed  children  in  two  situations,  one  free  play  and 
one  alone  with  the  experimenter,  and  recorded  their  use  of  pro- 
nouns. She  also  found  gradual  increases  in  the  use  of  pronouns 
with  age.  However,  Goodenough  further  observed  that 


178  THE  GENETIC  FORMATION  OF  THE  EGO 

Pronouns  of  the  first  person  singular  occur  with  far  greater  fre- 
quency during  free  play  with  other  children  than  in  the  controlled 
situation  where  the  child  presumably  feels  less  need  to  assert  himself. 
The  same  trend  is  shown  in  the  use  of  the  possessives,  my  and  mine. 
[23,  337] 

Here  we  see  the  striking  influence  of  factors  coming  from  specific 
situations  upon  behavior  regulated  by  the  ego.  Although  we  shall 
consider  this  topic  in  greater  detail  later  in  this  chapter,  it  should 
be  noted  here  that  the  results  of  functional  analyses  of  behavior 
such  as  those  suggested  by  Piaget  (for  example,  coefficients  of 
"egocentric"  behavior)  will  surely  vary,  within  limits,  according 
to  the  situation  in  which  the  child  is  behaving. 

THE  EFFECTS  OF  CERTAIN  SOCIAL  FACTORS  ON  EGO  FORMATION 

In  this  section,  we  shall  call  attention  to  some  representative 
studies  which  deal  specifically  with  the  effects  of  age-mate  groups. 
This  subject  has  been  badly  neglected  by  some  sociologists  who 
maintain  that  all  ego-values  are  handed  down  only  by  the  well- 
established  order  of  the  adult  generation  at  any  given  period. 
Without  attempting  to  give  a  comprehensive  summary  of  work  in 
this  field,  we  cite  these  studies  to  illustrate  the  influence  of  age- 
mate  contacts  on  the  process  of  ego  formation.  In  some  of  the 
studies  on  the  early  collective  behavior  and  play  group  activities 
of  children,  there  are  indications  that  the  situation  often  can  and 
does  determine  such  interpersonal  characteristics  as  aggressive  or 
co-operative  behavior,  referred  to  by  some  psychologists  as  "per- 
sonality traits."  In  the  studies  of  Lois  Murphy  [47]  and  Jersild 
[34],  to  be  summarized,  we  find,  for  example,  evidence  that  these 
"personality  traits"  are  neither  absolute  and  unchanging  properties 
of  the  individual,  nor  fixed  characteristics  formed  in  early  infancy. 
Any  theory  of  personality  will  gain  much  breadth,  in  our  opinion, 
if  the  situational  determinations  and  fluctuations,  within  certain 
limits,  of  course,  are  given  the  consideration  they  deserve.  Con- 
temporary child  psychologists  have  stressed  the  importance  of 
situational  factors.  Thus  Stoddard  and  Wellman  [61],  summariz- 
ing certain  considerations  involved  in  observing  the  social  behavior 
of  children,  comment  that  one  is  forced  "to  accept  the  conclusion 


THE  EFFECTS  OF  CERTAIN  SOCIAL  FACTORS  ON  EGO  FORMATION     179 

that  the  amount  of  behavior  exhibited  may  be  so  contingent  upon 
particularized  conditions  that  consistency  from  day  to  day  or  from 
week  to  week  is  unusual."  [257]  Of  course,  some  situations,  such 
as  games  continuing  from  day  to  day,  perhaps  for  weeks,  or  rela- 
tive roles  taken  by  adolescent  boys  or  girls  in  clique  situations,  may 
leave  their  imprint  on  the  individual  members  to  a  greater  or 
lesser  degree. 

We  shall  also  refer  here  briefly  to  the  effect  of  values  or  norms 
of  the  established  social  order  usually  handed  down  in  the  form  of 
short-cut  dictums.  Investigators  like  Wallon,  Salusky,  and  Davis 
have  called  attention  with  accumulating  evidence  to  the  effect  of 
material  and  technological  surroundings  on  the  developing  iden- 
tity of  the  individual.  The  general  technological  level  of  the 
milieu,  the  amount  and  kind  of  toys  and  games,  the  size  of  living 
space,  the  kind  and  amount  of  nourishment,  the  kinds  of  material 
opportunities,  and  the  like,  as  well  as  particular  values  or  norms 
are  all  important  parts  of  the  child's  outside  world.  In  the  next 
three  chapters  we  shall  consider  further  the  effect  of  such  factors 
in  determining  or  influencing  group  identifications  and  loyalties. 

Some  effects  of  age-mate  groups.  Especially  since  the  classical 
work  of  Piaget  on  children's  play,  many  studies  have  been  con- 
cerned with  the  implications  of  the  child's  participation  in  collec- 
tive activities.  These  activities,  of  course,  require  the  observance 
of  certain  rules  and  the  conformity  to  specific  roles  relative  to 
other  individuals.  From  these  studies  we  see  that  there  is  a  pro- 
gressive development  in  the  social  participation  of  the  child  with 
other  children.  This  progressive  development  is  observed  in  the 
child's  increased  capacity  to  take  part  in  play  and  to  join  as  a 
member  of  an  age-mate  group.  The  development  is  also  revealed 
by  the  increasing  duration  of  collective  activity,  the  increasing  size 
of  the  group  (that  is,  the  number  of  participants),  and  the  increas- 
ing complexity  of  the  rules  to  be  observed  in  particular  group 
situations.  All  of  these  reciprocal  relationships  imply  that  the 
growing  child  has  to  learn  that  he  has  definite  relationships  to 
other  children  and  persons. 

It  is  only  after  a  child  learns  his  own  position  relative  to  other 
individuals  around  him  that  he  can  successfully  participate  in 
group  activities.  And  only  after  such  a  realization  can  the  child 


180  THE  GENETIC  FORMATION  OF  THE  EGO 

carry  on  the  more  or  less  consistent  behavior  demanded  by  lasting 
social  relationships  which  involve  definite  status,  values,  loyalties, 
responsibilities.  Important  implications  for  ego  development  can 
be  derived  from  investigations  of  age-mate  groups.  With  the  in- 
creased interest  in  the  development  of  the  social  behavior  of  chil- 
dren, significant  formulations  concerned  with  the  implications  of 
studies  of  group  participation  are  beginning  to  appear  in  general 
textbooks  on  child  psychology. 

For  example,  Goodenough  [24],  in  her  Developmental  Psy- 
chology (1945),  traces  the  social  development  of  the  child  from  the 
rather  solitary  play  activities  of  early  childhood  to  the  stage  when 
he  is  absorbed  in  group  games  with  age  mates  with  whom  he 
identifies  himself: 

The  four-year-old  still  says,  "I  want  someone  to  play  with  me" 
But  a  year  or  so  later  comes  the  dawn  of  a  new  social  concept.  Now 
we  more  often  hear,  "I  want  to  go  and  play  with  the  other  children!' 
The  child  no  longer  sees  himself  purely  as  an  individual  but  is 
beginning  to  identify  himself  with  the  group.  [24,  394  /.,  italics  in  last 
sentence  ours] 

Again,  Hurlock  [30],  in  Child  Development  (1942),  gives  a 
similar  picture: 

After  the  child  has  entered  school  and  has  come  in  contact  with 
other  children,  he  loses  interest  in  playing  around  the  house,  alone, 
or  with  one  or  two  companions.  He  likewise  now  considers  it  a  bore 
and  not  a  treat  to  accompany  his  parents  on  picnics,  parties,  or  family 
gatherings.  At  the  same  time,  interest  in  individual  games  gives  way 
to  group  games,  and  play  without  companions  loses  its  charm.  The 
child  has  entered  the  "gang  age,"  an  age  when  social  consciousness 
develops  very  rapidly. 

The  child's  gang  is  a  result  of  a  spontaneous  effort  on  the  part  of 
the  child  to  create  a  society  adequate  to  meet  his  needs.  .  .  .  Through 
gang  influences,  the  child  receives  important  training  in  social  be- 
havior that  could  not  be  obtained  with  comparable  success  under 
conditions  imposed  by  adult  society.  There  is  an  awakening  of  social 
consciousness  at  this  time  which  is  fundamental  to  all  social  behavior. 
[30,233] 

Without  taking  sides  as  to  whether  or  not  there  is  a  "gang  age," 
a  brief  investigation  of  the  formation  of  age-mate  groups  and  the 


THE  EFFECTS  OF  CERTAIN  SOCIAL  FACTORS  ON  EGO  FORMATION     181 

frequently  reported  absorption  of  children  in  such  groups  may 
lead  us  to  a  better  understanding  of  the  function  of  values  merely 
imposed  from  above  and  those  generated  in  situations  of  active 
participation.  During  the  years  1926  and  1927,  Parten  [49-51] 
conducted  extensive  studies  on  the  development  of  social  behavior 
in  children  from  two  to  five  years  of  age  in  free  play  situations. 
Classifying  children's  play  behavior  into  the  categories  "unoccu- 
pied behavior,"  "onlooker  behavior,"  "solitary  play,"  "parallel 
play,"  "associative  group  play,"  and  "cooperative  group  play,"  she 
found  that  only  25  per  cent  of  the  behavior  fell  into  unsocial 
categories  (the  first  three) ;  all  the  other  observations  were  of  social 
behavior  (the  last  three  types).  Even  more  important  for  our 
present  consideration  is  the  close  relationship  between  the  type  of 
play  activity  and  age  of  the  children.  She  found  that 

All  the  correlations  between  age  and  the  unsocial  play  types  are 
high  and  negative,  and  all  those  between  age  and  socially  organized 
play  are  high  and  positive.  .  .  .  Only  the  youngest  children,  those 
from  two  to  three  years,  were  found  unoccupied  during  the  sixty 
observations.  Solitary  play  was  most  common  at  two  and  one-half 
years  but  there  is  a  decided  decline  in  the  importance  of  solitary  play 
at  three  and  again  at  four  years.  Onlookers  were  most  prevalent 
among  the  two-and-one-half  to  three-year-olds.  .  .  .  The  oldest  chil- 
dren do  not  engage  in  onlooking  behavior  frequently.  If  an  activity 
interests  them  they  want  to  participate  in  it.  They  know  all  the 
techniques  of  securing  entrance  into  a  group,  so  do  not  remain  ob- 
servers for  any  noticeable  length  of  time.  Parallel  play  groups  were 
observed  most  often  among  the  two-year-olds,  and  least  often  among 
the  children  from  three  to  four.  As  children  became  older,  they 
invariably  conversed  with  one  another  about  their  activities,  and  be- 
came interested  in  their  associates.  Associative  group  play  increased 
in  popularity  as  the  children  became  older,  and  was  most  frequent 
in  the  oldest  group.  There  is  a  marked  increase  in  organized  sup- 
plementary play  beginning  with  the  third  year.  This  sudden  interest 
in  cooperative^play  is  perhaps  accounted  for  by  the  popularity  of  the 
activity  of  "play  house"  among  the  three-year-olds.  Since  the  young 
children  lac\  the  power  of  expressing  themselves  with  language,  they 
have  difficulty  in  playing  in  cooperative  groups.  [49,  265  /.,  italics 
ours] 


182  THE  GENETIC  FORMATION  OF  THE  EGO 

Because  co-operative  group  play  is  found  so  predominantly 
among  older  children,  and  because  its  appearance  is  so  closely 
associated  with  the  development  of  leaders,  it  is  important  that 
we  examine  Parten's  description  of  these  groups  more  closely. 
Such  groups  are  organized  for  the  purpose  of  making  something, 
dramatizing  situations,  playing  formal  games,  and  so  on. 

There  is  a  marked  sense  of  belonging  or  of  not  belonging  to  the 
group.  The  control  of  the  group  situation  is  in  the  hands  of  one  or 
two  of  the  members  who  direct  the  activity  of  the  others.  [49,  251, 
italics  ours] 

Different  roles  are  taken  by  the  various  group  members,  and  the 
activity  is  organized  "so  that  the  efforts  of  one  child  are  supple- 
mented by  those  of  another."  [257]  Before  a  child  can  participate 
in  such  a  group,  a  certain  degree  of  ego  development  must  take 
place  so  that  the  child  can  see  himself  in  some  definite  relationship 
to  the  other  children  as  required  by  the  group  situation.  Parten 
[50]  found  that,  although  in  the  older  age  groups  some  children 
had  a  more  or  less  lasting  leadership  role  even  with  "gang"  or- 
ganization, most  of  the  children  engaged  in  both  directing  and 
following  activity.  Leadership,  then,  was  seen  to  be  a  function  of 
the  group  and  its  activities  as  well  as  a  junction  of  the  individual 
child.  She  also  found  that  as  the  year  in  nursery  school  went  on, 
more  leadership  behavior  occurred.  During  the  last  two  thirds  of 
the  year,  individual  differences  in  leadership  became  more  out- 
standing. 

Apparently  the  middle  period  was  one  in  which  the  children  found 
their  places,  and  cumulative  experience  in  these  positions  enabled  each 
child  to  hold  his  place  as  a  leader  or  follower  thereafter  with  a  mini- 
mum of  effort  and  resistance  from  others.  [50,  440,  italics  ours] 

Consistent  with  the  foregoing  results  was  the  finding  that  the 
size  of  the  groups  in  which  children  participated  tended  to  increase 
with  age.  Older  children  had  learned  definite  ways  of  gaining 
entrance  to  a  group,  with  remarks  such  as  "Can*!  play  too?"  or 
"Shall  I  be  the  little  brother?",  while  the  group  members  also 
sometimes  invited  a  child  to  join  them.  [51, 137] 

'As  notions  of  the  child's  self  in  relation  to  other  children  develop 
progressively  with  age,  a  corresponding  change  is  seen  in  his 


THE  EFFECTS  OF  CERTAIN  SOCIAL  FACTORS  ON  EGO  FORMATION     183 

manipulation  of  and  interest  in  toys  and  other  play  material.  The 
favorite  activities  and  toys  of  older  children  tended  to  be  ones 
involving  the  interaction  of  several  children.  Also,  "younger  and 
older  children  differ  in  the  manner  in  which  they  play  with  toys 
and  hence  in  the  social  values  the  toy  has  for  them."  [147]  For 
example,  a  young  child's  play  in  the  sandpile  seldom  involved 
social  intercourse,  however,  whereas  the  older  children  more  often 
played  in  the  sand  together  constructing  houses,  roads,  and  such. 

Bridges'  investigations  [6]  also  show  the  necessity  of  ego  de- 
velopment for  the  understanding  of  the  reciprocal  relationships 
required  for  participation  in  age-mate  groups.  She  concluded 
that  qualitative  differences  between  the  social  development  of 
young  and  older  preschool  children  were  best  explained  in  terms 
of  their  consideration  of  themselves  in  relation  to  the  other  chil- 
dren. 

Two-year-olds  usually  play  or  work  by  themselves  with  little  refer- 
ence to  others  except  to  claim  their  toys  or  otherwise  interfere  with 
them.  [82] 

Older  children  engage  more  often  in  group  play  than  younger  ones 
and  seldom  play  alone.  .  .  .  They  usually  wait  their  turn  for  little 
treats  and  duties  in  contrast  to  the  two-year-olds  who  always  want  to 
be  first.  ...  In  short,  between  the  ages  of  two  and  five  years  children 
in  nursery  school  progress  from  being  socially  indifferent  infants, 
through  the  stages  of  self-assertiveness  and  interference  with  the  liber- 
ties of  others,  to  a  stage  in  which  they  show  consideration,  sympathy 
and  kindness  for  others.  [6,  85] 

In  Lois  Murphy's  observations  of  child  behavior,  we  have  one 
more  index  of  the  development  of  the  experiences  of  one's  self  in 
relation  to  others.  From  the  developmental  point  of  view,  Murphy 
summarizes  her  findings  as  follows: 

Statistical  analysis  clothes  in  more  definite  terms  the  impression  we 
recorded  earlier,  that  among  children  from  two  to  four  years  of  age 
in  a  nursery-school  situation,  sympathetic  behavior  increases  with 
chronological  age,  with  mental  age,  and  with  intelligence  measured 
in  terms  of  LQ.  Bridges'  descriptions  of  the  behavior  of  children  in 
a  Toronto  nursery  school  anticipated  this,  and  there  are  no  data  from 
any  other  investigation  which  could  lead  us  to  question  this  conclu- 
sion. [47, 171] 


184  THE  GENETIC  FORMATION  OF  THE  EGO 

It  should  be  noted  that  correlations  of  sympathetic  behavior  with 
mental  age  were  substantially  lower  than  those  with  chronological 
age.  As  Murphy  observes,  the  investigations  of  Bridges,  Parten, 
and  Beaver  clearly  indicated  that 

The  beginning  of  social  response  in  groups  like  these  frequently 
takes  the  form  of  merely  watching,  then  later  of  playing  alongside 
another  child.  Neither  of  these  stages  involves  overt  give  and  take; 
this  begins  sometimes  with  physical  contact  and  sometimes  with  verbal 
contact,  depending  on  the  resources  of  the  child.  [47,  51] 

In  other  words,  in  the  early  entrance  into  situations  with  other 
children,  reciprocal  relationships  are  infrequently  observed.  In 
terms  of  sympathetic  behavior,  the  focal  point  of  Murphy's  inves- 
tigation, she  finds  that  the 

.  .  .  sequence  of  responses,  appearing  from  one  age  to  another,  seems 
to  be:  (1)  staring  (paying  attention  to  the  distress  of  another  child); 
(2)  asking  about,  commenting  on,  and  so  forth  (except  in  the  case 
of  markedly  nonverbal  children);  (3)  active  responses  of  comfort, 
help,  defense,  and  the  like.  [47,  152] 

Clearly,  the  child  recognizes  another  child  to  be  a  person  like 
himself  before  the  latter  type  of  response  occurs. 

In  this  connection,  Murphy  points  out  that  a  very  young  child 
will  respond  emotionally  to  attacks  on  his  mother  or  other  persons 
who  are  close  to  him,  but  that  at  this  early  stage  such  behavior  is 
not  chiefly  due  to  identification  with  that  other  person  but  to  "lack 
of  discrimination  between  self  and  others,  or  between  self  and  the 
rest  of  the  world."  [296]  This  is  confirmation  40  years  later  of 
Baldwin's  observation  that  the  small  child's  ego  "includes  too 
much." 

After  differentiation  "between  self  and  others'*  is  clear,  sympa- 
thetic behavior  occurs  "in  varying  degree  in  response  to  different 
individuals  and  is  affected  by  variations  in  threshold  due  to  differ- 
ences in  identifications  with  different  individuals,"  [303]  For 
example: 

In  Group  P  we  have  commented  upon  episodes  of  response  within 
a  small  gang,  of  which  Louis  was  the  leader  and  Stephen,  the  young- 
est, was  the  most  gang-conscious  member.  Stephen's  concern  for 


THE  EFFECTS  OF  CERTAIN  SOCIAL  FACTORS  ON  EGO  FORMATION     185 

Louis  and  Henry  was  dependable,  although  he  seldom  shared  concern 
for  any  other  child.  George  was  usually  more  sympathetic  with  his 
twin  brother  than  with  anyone  at  issue  with  the  brother.  [47,  314] 

Further  indication  that  sympathetic  responses  occur  in  terms  of 
the  child's  relation  to  other  children  in  the  group  is  found,  for 
example,  in  Murphy's  comment  that 

Friends,  younger  children,  favorites  of  the  group,  are  most  likely  to 
receive  a  relatively  large  number  of  sympathetic  responses.  [47,  159, 
italics  ours] 

Sympathetic  behavior  was  also  seen  to  be  dependent  upon  the 
relative  stability  and  the  character  of  reciprocal  relationships 
within  the  group. 

One  of  the  most  interesting  illustrations  of  the  possibility  of  a  quick 
shift  of  attitude  and  expression  in  reaction  to  a  shift  in  ego-status, 
appeared  in  a  sad  drama  of  hurt  egos,  where  Janet's  usual  consistent 
friendliness  and  sympathy  turned  to  aggressive  retaliation,  after  she 
felt  she  had  been  intentionally  injured  by  Heidi,  one  of  her  closest 
friends.  [47, 180,  italics  ours] 

In  the  subsequent  protocol,  it  is  clear  that  restoration  of  the  usual 
group  activity  was  achieved  not  merely  by  the  two  children  in- 
volved, but  through  the  intervention  and  comfort  of  other  group 
members.  This  is  just  one  example  in  which  "sympathetic  re- 
sponses, which  ordinarily  appear  in  certain  situations,  are  inhib- 
ited when  the  ego  of  the  potentially  responsive  child  is  threat- 
ened." [181]  The  investigation  also  indicated  that  a  child  might 
not  react  positively  to  a  situation  quite  amusing  to  others  when  he 
(or  his  things)  were  highly  involved  in  the  "joke."  [87] 

We  would  not  be  doing  justice  to  Murphy's  results  if  we  over- 
looked her  emphasis  on  the  influence  of  material  and  social  fac- 
tors from  the  grown-up  world  on  the  development  and  frequency 
of  sympathetic  and  co-operative  behavior.  For  example,  conflict 
was  found  to  be  greater  if  less  play  space  and  fewer  toys  were 
available;  somewhat  more  sympathetic  responses  were  found  for 
children  of  lower  socioeconomic  status;  and  adult  values  and 
norms  were  seen  as  potent  influences  on  the  children's  social  be- 
havior. 


186  THE  GENETIC  FORMATION  OF  THE  EGO 

Murphy's  report  of  the  influence  on  behavior  of  relationships 
within  a  group  leads  to  a  further  inquiry  into  the  relationships  of 
children  to  their  age  mates.  Generally  speaking,  investigations  of 
factors  related  to  friendship  or  companionship  among  young  chil- 
dren have  been  relatively  unprofitable,  since  they  have  usually 
attempted  only  to  correlate  characteristics,  abilities,  and  similar 
traits  of  young  friends  and  have  overlooked  group  factors.  Little 
relationship  between  the  intelligence,  physical  development,  and 
similar  traits  of  childhood  friends  has  been  found.  However, 
Furfey's  study  [18]  indicates  that  proximity,  that  is,  being  in  the 
same  neighborhood  or  the  same  classroom,  is  an  important  factor 
in  the  friendship  of  young  children.  Similarly,  Hagman  [26] 
found  that  preschool  children  who  associated  outside  of  as  well 
as  in  school  tended  to  be  close  companions.  As  a  criterion  of 
friendship  among  preschool  children,  Challman  [9]  used  the  num- 
ber of  times  a  child  was  in  a  group  containing  another  given  child. 
With  this  criterion,  higher  relationships  than  usual  were  found 
between  the  chronological  ages  and  ratings  of  sociality  of  friends. 

Studies  of  the  "popularity"  of  young  children  in  the  eyes  of 
their  age  mates  are  somewhat  more  revealing  as  indications  of  the 
relationship  of  one  child  to  others.  Some  of  the  significant  find- 
ings of  such  studies  are:  one's  ideas  of  one's  self  seem  to  be  more 
closely  related  to  the  opinions  of  age  mates  than  to  those  of  adults 
(teachers  in  this  case)  [41];  less  "popular"  children  tend  to  be 
less  sure  of  their  relationships  to  others  in  the  group  [36];  and 
relative  status  (popularity),  once  established,  tends  to  be  more  or 
less  lasting  while  the  group  is  together.  [5] 

Returning  now  to  studies  of  the  social  behavior  of  young  chil- 
dren in  general,  we  find  several  which  illustrate  the  progressive 
development  of  children's  social  relationships  with  age  mates. 
Berne  [4]  found  significant  differences  between  older  children 
(three-  and  four-year-olds)  and  younger  children  (two-year-olds) 
on  a  "social  behavior  rating  scale"  for  such  items  as  "interested  in 
the  group,"  "socially  conformant,"  "sociable,"  "irresponsible  for 
self,"  "irresponsible  for  others."  In  the  latter  items  the  older  chil- 
dren were  rated  more  responsible  than  the  younger.  These  dif- 
ferences were  also  observed  in  experimental  situations.  For  ex- 
ample, Berne  measured  co-operation  by  placing  two  children  in 


THE  EFFECTS  OF  CERTAIN  SOCIAL  FACTORS  ON  EGO  FORMATION      187 

a  room  with  one  group  of  toys.  Co-operation  was  scored  in  terms 
of  the  amount  of  time  spent  playing  with  the  other  child.  Co- 
operative behavior,  as  so  measured,  tended  to  increase  with  age. 
Evidence  that  children's  social  relationships  become  increasingly 
complex  with  age  is  found  also  in  studies  of  social  contacts,  such 
as  those  of  Beaver  [3]  who  observed  that  the  number  of  social 
contacts  in  play  situations  tends  to  increase  with  age.  Green  [25] 
found  that  the  time  spent  in  group  play  and  the  size  of  the  group 
involved  tended  to  increase  with  age. 

To  stress  the  fact  that  children's  participation  in  group  games 
increases  progressively  with  age  in  terms  of  the  duration  of  the 
play  activity  and  the  size  of  the  group  may  seem  to  be  stressing 
a  truism  of  common  sense.  However,  in  view  of  the  emphasis 
sometimes  placed  on  "sociability,"  extraversion,  and  intraversion, 
as  inherent  personality  traits,  the  implications  of  the  ^  progressive 
development  of  participation  in  group  activities  are  important. 
These  participations  may  serve  as  an  index  of  developing  sociabil- 
ity to  which  situational  factors  contribute  so  significantly.  And 
such  facts  as  these  must  be  emphasized  if  we  are  to  get  an  accurate 
picture  of  the  way  a  person  acquires  his  sense  of  social  identity. 
For  example,  reversals  in  the  characteristics  of  social  behavior  have 
been  experimentally  demonstrated.  Jack  [33]  investigated  "as- 
cendant" behavior  in  preschool  children,  ascendant  behavior  being 
defined  in  terms  of  the  pursuit  of  a  certain  activity  even  against 
the  interference  and  directing  behavior  of  others  and  attempts  to 
control  the  behavior  of  others.  Finding  that  "self-confidence"  or 
lack  of  it  seemed  to  be  the  most  outstanding  difference  between 
the  most  ascendant  and  nonascendant  children,  Jack  trained  the 
five  most  nonascendant  children  by  giving  them  information  about 
and  chances  to  use  the  play  materials.  When  again  placed  in  the 
experimental  situation,  these  subjects  showed  large  and  signifi- 
cant gains  in  the  amount  of  ascendant  behavior.  A  similar  study 
was  made  by  Page  [48]  using  Jack's  methods.  Page  summarizes 
her  principal  findings  as  follows: 

The  data  which  have  been  presented  offer  proof  of  the  modifiability 
of  ascendant  behavior  in  nonascendant  and  moderately  ascendant 
three-  and  four-year-old  children.  This  was  accomplished  by  training 


188  THE  GENETIC  FORMATION  OF  THE  EGO 

designed  to  increase  feelings  of  self-confidence.  Tentative,  but  strongly 
suggestive,  evidence  is  given  that  the  effects  of  these  training  proce- 
dures are  transferred  to  ordinary  preschool  situations.  [48,  50] 

The  investigations  reviewed  indicate  further  that  the  develop- 
ment of  social  behavior  seems  also  to  be  dependent  upon  the 
amount  and  kind  of  contact  with  age  mates.  The  results  of  Jer- 
sild  and  Fite  [35]  show  that  "children  who  have  had  previous 
nursery  school  experience  enter  into  a  decidedly  larger  average 
number  of  social  contacts  than  do  children  who  have  not  pre- 
viously attended  nursery  school."  [100]  This  was  found  even 
when  contacts  made  by  children  in  the  "old"  group  with  their 
special  companions  were  discounted.  However,  during  the  year, 
the  "new"  children  increased  their  social  contacts  from  only  54 
per  cent  as  many  as  those  of  the  "old"  to  80  per  cent  as  many  con- 
tacts as  made  by  the  "old"  group.  "The  opportunities  for  social 
participation  afforded  by  the  nursery  school  do  not,  according  to 
the  present  findings,  have  the  effect  of  submerging  the  child's  own 
individuality."  [108]  Pointing  to  the  necessity  of  appraising  such 
data  "in  terms  of  its  context"  or  situation,  the  authors  describe 
the  behavior  of  several  children.  These  examples  indicate  that 
an  increase  or  decrease  in  social  contact  is  closely  related  to  ego 
experiences  coming  from  the  stimulation  of  the  group. 

One  of  the  children  in  question  made  a  very  low  score  on  social 
contacts  in  the  fall.  About  half  of  these  contacts  were  aggressive: 
the  child  "went  after"  other  children,  and  conflicts  ensued.  His  spring 
records  showed  a  threefold  increase  in  social  contacts,  evidences  of 
greater  security  and  enjoyment  of  the  company  of  other  children  and 
of  the  nursery  school  situation,  and  a  decided  drop  in  the  frequency 
of  his  conflicts.  Another  child  likewise  made  a  low  score  in  social 
contacts  in  the  fall,  and  was  involved  in  scarcely  any  conflicts  at  all. 
She  was  withdrawn  and  yielding.  In  the  spring,  she  showed  a  sixfold 
increase  in  social  contacts,  greater  security  and  enjoyment  of  the  com- 
pany of  other  children,  and,  with  this,  an  increase  in  frequency  of 
conflicts.  Her  conflicts  took  the  form  of  protecting  her  own  interests 
and  play  activities,  and  of  warding  off  the  attentions  of  a  child  who 
previously  had  sought  to  dominate  her.  The  child's  improved  socia- 
bility, in  this  case,  was  accompanied  by,  and  to  some  extent  facilitated 


THE  EFFECTS  OF  CERTAIN  SOCIAL  FACTORS  ON  EGO  FORMATION     189 

by,  her  readiness  to  rise  to  her  own  defense  in  carrying  out  her 
interests  and  in  joining  in  the  play  of  several  children.  In  the  third 
case,  a  boy  who  entered  into  a  large  number  of  social  contacts  in  the 
fall  exhibited  a  decrease  in  social  contacts  in  the  spring,  and  with  this 
decrease  there  was  an  increase  in  relative  frequency  of  fighting.  This 
boy  .  .  .  had  lost  his  hold  on  a  child  whom  he  had  dominated  in  the 
fall;  his  records  reveal  that  this  loss  was  followed  by  signs  of  insecurity 
and  loss  of  self-confidence.  .  .  .  [35, 103] 

A  study  carried  out  in  the  Soviet  Union  by  Salusky  [58]  reveals 
the  same  general  trend  of  social  development  through  increased 
group  contacts  and  participations.  This  study  typifies  the  appli- 
cation of  dialectical  method  and  is  particularly  important  because 
of  the  clue  it  gives  for  the  study  of  any  grouping  (or  collective) 
on  the  human  level  as  contrasted  to  the  study  of  interactions  on 
inorganic  or  organic  levels.  Defining  the  collective  as  "a  group 
of  persons  between  whom  is  observed  an  interaction,  a  group 
which  reacts  as  a  whole  to  a  given  situation"  [365],  Salusky  com- 
ments that  interactions  "of  a  social  order  have  their  own  specific 
qualities,  distinguishing  them  both  from  the  interaction  of  un- 
organic  particles  and  from  the  interaction  of  organic  systems  and 
organic  elements."  [375] 

In  human  associations  we  observe  the  interaction  of  the  highest  type. 
The  best  way  of  observing  their  genesis  is  in  observation  of  children's 
collectives.  [58,  368] 

Collectives  may  be  spontaneous  as  in  associations  for  play  when 
the  games  originate  with  the  children,  or  they  may  be  organized, 
as  in  the  case  of  daily  meetings  of  homeless  children.  In  either 
case,  they  may  be  of  longer  or  shorter  duration.  Salusky  believes 
that  all  types  of  such  associations  must  be  studied  with  a  single 
systematic  plan. 

For  our  present  discussion  and  that  to  follow,  Salusky's  classifi- 
cation of  stimuli  is  particularly  important.  Behavior  in  the  col- 
lectives is  determined  by  exogenous  as  well  as  endogenous  stimuli, 
the  former  coining  from  the  environment,  the  latter  from  the 
children  and  within  the  group  itself.  Mentioning  first  some  im- 
portant endogenous  stimuli,  Salusky  reports  results  of  studies  made 
in  three  districts  of  Ukrania.  He  finds  a  "basic  tendency"  that 


190  THE  GENETIC  FORMATION  OF  THE  EGO 

"associations  of  older  children  are  of  longer  life  than  associations 
of  younger  children.  It  is  only  a  tendency,  not  a  general  law." 
[371]  The  duration  of  collectives  depends 

.  .  .  largely  upon  the  character  of  the  game.  The  latter  is  deter- 
mined by  the  child's  stock  of  experience  and  by  the  degree  of  forma- 
tion of  his  anatomic  and  physiological  development.  [58,  370] 

These  factors  are  dependent  to  a  large  extent  upon  his  age,  but 
are  also  influenced  by  the  milieu.  Salusky  noted  a  tendency  for  the 
number  of  children  associated  in  collectives  to  increase  with  age. 
[371]  However,  as  American  studies  have  also  shown,  association 
in  larger  groups  does  occur,  for  a  time  at  least,  in  the  younger 
years. 

Finally,  Salusky  shows  the  influence  of  the  milieu  upon  chil- 
dren's collectives.  Two  kindergartens  in  Kiev  were  observed. 
One  group  of  children  came  chiefly  from  homes  of  qualified  work- 
ers who  lived  a  modern  mode  of  life  in  an  industrial  area,  were 
socially  active,  read  newspapers  and  books.  The  second  group 
came  chiefly  from  parents  who  were  "unskilled  workers,  small 
tradesmen,  and  in  general,  the  proletariat"  where  the  old  mode 
of  life  prevailed.  Salusky  shows  the  difference  in  the  kind  of 
games  played  by  children  from  these  two  environments.  Whereas 
nearly  half  of  the  children  in  the  second  group  participated  in 
games  representing  the  old  mode  of  life,  only  about  six  per  cent 
of  the  children  in  the  first  group  participated  in  such  games.  A 
good  share  of  the  children  coming  from  modern  homes  played 
games  dramatizing  the  new  way  of  life  and  modern  technological 
developments,  whereas  children  who  lived  the  old  way  of  life  did 
not  participate  at  all  in  such  games. 

This  study  illustrates,  in  Salusky's  words,  that 

The  behavior  of  the  collective  is  determined  by  the  children's 
environment  and  by  the  source  of  their  experience.  The  stimuli  of  a 
situation  act  as  the  exciters.  [58,  576] 

This  point  will  help  us  in  understanding  the  character  and  direc- 
tion of  the  activities  of  spontaneously  formed  boys'  gangs  to  be 
discussed  in  chapter  10. 


THE  EFFECTS  OF  CERTAIN  SOCIAL  FACTORS  ON  EGO  FORMATION      191 

Ego  constituents  derived  from  established  society.  As  a  result 
of  participation  in  age-mate  groups,  children  incorporate  into 
themselves  certain  values  which  form  their  appropriate  attitudes. 
We  have  previously  pointed  out  the  special  importance  of  those 
attitudes  produced  by  the  inculcation  of  values  by  grown-ups, 
usually  in  the  form  of  short-cut  dictums.  For  these  eventually 
form  attitudes  that  define  in  a  more  or  less  lasting  way  the  status 
and  position  of  the  individual  in  his  community,  in  his  class,  in 
a  "race"  or  nationality  group,  or,  in  short,  in  society  at  large. 
Such  values  pertaining  to  interfamily  and  class  relationships, 
handed  down  by  parents  and  other  grown-ups  in  the  immediate 
surroundings  of  the  child,  result  in  attitudes  which  are  certainly 
not  discrete  unrelated  items  in  the  psychological  make-up  of  the 
individual,  but  which  become  important  constituents  of  the  ego. 

Further  experimental  evidence  can  be  mentioned  here  to  sub- 
stantiate this  point.  For  example,  the  studies  of  Ruth  Horowitz 
[28,  29]  and  M.  K.  and  K.  B.  Clark  [10,  11]  show  that  racial 
identifications  and  values,  developing  into  lasting  attitudes,  are 
handed  down  by  adults;  and,  further,  that,  if  the  child  is  to  grasp 
the  realistic  grim  meaning  of  these  values  to  be  incorporated  as 
lasting  attitudes,  an  appropriate  degree  of  ego  development  is  re- 
quired. Such  development  must  precede  the  child's  grasp  of  the 
realistic  (as  contrasted  to  the  earlier  autistic)  scheme  of  social  re- 
lationships, whatever  this  scheme  may  be  in  a  particular  society. 
In  this  connection,  we  add,  with  larger  society  in  view,  that  the 
grown-ups  involved  in  this  process  are  usually  transmitting  the 
values  or  norms  which  prevail  in  society  at  large,  or  in  their  par- 
ticular section  of  society. 

Horowitz  investigated  24  children  in  a  public  nursery  school, 
ranging  in  age  from  two  to  five  years.  [28]  Some  of  the  children 
were  white,  some  Negro.  In  Horowitz'  words,  the  study 

...  is  limited  to  children's  emergent  awareness  of  themselves,  with 
reference  to  a  specific  social  grouping.  It  deals  with  the  beginnings 
of  race-consciousness  conceived  as  a  function  of  ego-development.  [28, 
91] 

Various  pictures  of  white  and  Negro  children  were  shown,  and 
the  child  was  then  asked  to  tell  which  child  in  the  picture  was 


192  THE  GENETIC  FORMATION  OF  THE  EGO 

himself.  Horowitz  found  that,  on  the  average,  over  60  per  cent 
of  the  Negro  children  and  about  40  per  cent  of  the  white  children 
made  color  divisions  in  their  choices.  Commenting  on  this  differ- 
ence, she  says'  that,  for  the  Negro  children, 

It  is  as  if  the  contrast  there  presented  is  a  lesson  well  learned  and 
perceived  immediately  in  terms  of  its  pertinent  elements.  [28] 

She  traces  this  "lesson  well  learned"  to  the  "constriction  of  the 
adult  environment"  in  the  homes  of  the  Negro  children. 

In  this  same  study,  the  girls  were  asked  to  identify  both  them- 
selves and  their  brothers  or  playmates.  Finding  that  the  girls 
adhered  to  color  divisions  considerably  less  in  the  latter  case,  the 
investigator  suggests: 

One  is  evidently  clearer  about  one's  own  role  and  distinguishing 
characteristics  than  about  those  of  another  person.  [28,  98] 

Her  conclusion  is  that 

These  data  seem  to  point  to  the  concept  of  group  consciousness  and 
group  identification  as  an  intrinsic  aspect  of  ego-development  and  basic 
to  the  understanding  of  the  dynamics  of  attitude  function  in  the  adult 
personality.  Before  the  ego  has  been  completely  formed,  in  the  very 
process  of  becoming,  we  find  it  subtly  appropriating  a  visible  symbol 
that  has  been  socially  institutionalized  to  aid  it  in  its  work  of  marking 
itself  off  from  all  the  not-self  of  which  until  such  demarcation  has 
been  established  it  partakes.  This  may  mean,  however,  that  the  self 
is  defined  in  terms  that  make  successive  demarcations  necessary,  since 
in  the  perception  of  a  difference  an  assertion  of  identification  is  in- 
volved. The  individual's  attitude  toward  his  group  evidently  is  an 
integral  part  of  himself,  in  terms  of  which  he  is  fashioned,  under 
some  circumstances  of  life.  [28,  99] 

Horowitz  [29]  has  also  attempted  by  the  picture  technique  to 
study  other  identifications  of  preschool  children,  for  example,  age, 
size,  sex,  familial  position  or  status,  economic  status.  The  most 
correct  identifications  were  found  in  terms  of  sex  and  familial 
position.  Other  referents  produced  some  confusion,  for  interest- 
ing reasons.  For  example,  in  using  the  pictures  of  children  of 
different  sizes,  Horowitz  found  that  the  choice  depended  greatly 
on  whom  the  child  related  to  himself.  If  he  related  himself  to 


THE  EFFECTS  OF  CERTAIN  SOCIAL  FACTORS  ON  EGO  FORMATION     193 

"little  brother,"  he,  of  course,  chose  the  larger  of  the  children  as 
himself,  while,  if  to  a  "big  boy,"  he  chose  the  smaller.  The  other 
pictures  proved  to  be  somewhat  ambiguous  for  the  children  in  one 
way  or  another. 

Before  leaving  these  studies,  we  should  report  an  observation 
which  illustrates  the  relative  value  self-identifications  may  have 
for  a  young  child.  A  little  girl  who  had  beautiful  curly  hair  made 
color  distinctions  in  her  choices  rather  consistently  until  she  saw 
a  picture  in  which  the  Negro  child  had  curly  hair.  Although  she 
had  given  clear  evidence  of  recognizing  the  skin  differences  pre- 
viously, this  child,  whose  hair  had  undoubtedly  often  been  ad- 
mired, would  not  admit  any  skin  difference  in  this  case  and  clung 
to  her  choice  of  the  curly  haired  child. 

Continuing  along  the  lines  of  Horowitz'  provocative  study, 
K.  B.  and  M.  K.  Clark  [10]  studied  150  Negro  children  in  segre- 
gated nursery  schools.  Fifty  children  were  in  each  of  the  three-, 
four-,  and  five-year  age  groups.  The  technique  used  was  essen- 
tially the  same  as  Horowitz'.  Some  of  the  pictures  included  ani- 
mals. These  investigators  also  found  that  boys  adhered  to  color 
divisions  more  consistently  in  choosing  their  own  picture  than  did 
girls  who  were  asked  to  choose  their  brothers  or  playmates.  The 
resulting  choices  for  boys  showed  an  increase  in  the  percentage 
of  choices  of  Negro  children  from  31.5  per  cent  at  age  three  to 
63  per  cent  at  age  five.  In  the  three-year-old  group,  17.6  per  cent 
of  the  choices  were  of  animals.  Such  choices  did  not  occur  after 
three  years  of  age.  Some  five-year-olds  refused  to  make  choices, 
remarking,  "I'm  not  in  that  picture,"  or  "This  is  a  white  boy;  this 
is  a  colored  boy;  this  is  a  lion,"  and  so  on. 

Later,  these  same  investigators  conducted  an  ingenious  study 
[11]  using  as  subjects  children  of  the  same  age  groups,  33  of  whom 
were  of  light  coloring,  66  of  medium  coloring,  and  54  of  dark 
coloring.  As  they  stated: 

An  investigation  of  the  factors  inherent  in  the  genesis  of  racial 
identification  would  obviously  lead  to  an  understanding  of  the  dy- 
namics of  self -consciousness  and  its  social  determinants.  [11,  159] 

The  picture  technique  was  employed  again.  The  results  showed 
that  the  light  children  chose  a  Negro  child  in  the  pictures  in  36.5 


194  THE  GENETIC  FORMATION  OF  THE  EGO 

per  cent  of  their  total  choices,  the  medium  children  in  52.6  per 
cent,  and  the  dark  children  in  56.4  per  cent.  The  percentage  of 
choices  of  Negro  children  increased  for  the  color  groups  with  age. 
In  the  case  of  the  light  group,  the  increase  was  only  34.6  per  cent 
at  three  years  to  38.8  per  cent  at  five  years.  The  dark  group,  on 
the  other  hand,  increased  from  52,3  per  cent  at  three  years  to  70.1 
per  cent  at  five.  These  results  are  comparable  to  those  obtained 
by  Marks  in  his  study  of  the  judgments  of  adult  Negroes  of  vary- 
ing complexions,  (pp.  136 /.)  On  the  basis  of  these  results,  the 
Clarks  concluded: 

It  may  be  stated  that  consciousness  of  self  as  different  from  others 
on  the  basis  of  observed  skin  color  precedes  any  consciousness  of  self 
in  terms  of  socially  defined  group  differences  in  these  Negro  children. 
[11,  161] 

At  this  stage  it  appears  that  concepts  of  self  gleaned  from  the  con- 
crete physical  characteristics  of  perceived  self  become  modified  by 
social  factors,  taking  on  a  new  definition  in  the  light  of  these  social 
factors.  [11, 168] 

We  have  already  pointed  out  that  prejudices  are  not  so  much  the 
outcome  of  direct  personal  contacts  with  the  groups  toward  which 
the  prejudice  is  directed,  but  generally  are,  rather,  the  result  of 
contacts  with  the  attitudes  others  have  toward  these  groups.  The 
fact  has  been  recently  confirmed  by  Jersild  [34]  who  emphasizes 
the  influence  of  in-group  identifications  on  the  formation  of  these 
attitudes  which  become  so  important  in  defining  personal  identity. 
Jersild  believes  that  everyday  experiences  are  not  so  important  in 
the  acceptance  of  certain  attitudes  toward  various  groups  as  "the 
influences  that  come  at  second  hand,  the  prejudices  passed  on  to 
the  child  by  his  elders,  the  attitudes  that  he  comes  to  adopt  through 
precept  and  example  in  the  culture  that  surrounds  him."  [424] 

Frequently,  prejudice  thus  has  its  inception  in  the  home,  even 
though  neither  of  the  two  parents  openly  displays  any  antipathy  for 
another  group.  The  child  may  acquire  an  attitude  of  distrust  simply 
by  being  exposed  to  family  and  cultural  traditions  which  set  the  child 
and  his  people  off  from  others.  The  very  fact  that  a  child  is  made 
aware  of  the  national  origin  of  his  parents  means  that  he  is  being 
influenced  to  identify  himself  with  one  group  rather  than  another. 
[34,  425,  italics  ours] 


THE  EFFECTS  OF  CERTAIN  SOCIAL  FACTORS  ON  EGO  FORMATION     195 

Extensive  studies  by  Davis  [15,  16]  furnish  ample  evidence  that 
children's  attitudes  are  derived  from  the  material  conditions  and 
social  environments  in  which  they  develop.  These  attitudes  in- 
clude their  relative  standing  and  distance  with  regard  to  other 
children  of  different  classes,  their  lasting  likes  and  dislikes,  and 
their  views  on  major  issues  that  confront  them;  all  of  which  con- 
tribute to  the  formation  of  their  egos.  Davis'  generalizations, 
based  on  the  rich  material  at  his  command,  neatly  summarize  the 
influence  of  established  society  upon  the  developing  ego: 

The  number  of  class  controls  and  dogmas  which  a  child  must  learn 
and  struggle  continually  to  maintain,  in  order  to  meet  his  family's 
status  demands  as  a  class  unit,  is  great.  Class  training  of  the  child 
ranges  all  the  way  from  the  control  of  the  manner  and  ritual  by  which 
he  eats  his  food  to  the  control  of  his  choice  of  playmates  and  of  his 
educational  and  occupational  goals.  The  times  and  places  for  his 
recreation,  the  chores  required  of  him  by  his  family,  the  rooms  and 
articles  in  the  house  which  he  may  use,  the  wearing  of  certain  clothes 
at  certain  times,  the  amount  of  studying  required  of  him,  the  economic 
controls  to  which  he  is  subjected  by  his  parents,  indeed  his  very  con- 
ceptions of  right  and  wrong,  all  vary  according  to  the  social  class  of 
the  child  in  question.  Our  knowledge  of  social-class  training  and  of 
the  biological  and  psychological  differentials  in  child  development  as 
between  class  environments  is  now  sufficient  to  enable  us  to  say  that 
no  studies  can  henceforth  generalize  about  "the  child."  We  shall 
always  have  to  ask,  "A  child  of  what  class,  in  what  environment?" 
[15,6B9/.] 

In  this  chapter  we  have  reviewed  some  of  the  evidence  which 
so  firmly  establishes  the  fact  that  die  ego  is  formed  in  the  course 
of  genetic  development.  In  the  next  chapters,  dealing  with  ado- 
lescence, we  shall  see  how  the  ego  can  and  does  become  re-formed 
during  the  individual's  crucial  transition  from  child  to  adult.  And 
in  a  later  chapter  we  review  some  of  the  evidence  which  establishes 
the  further  fact  that  the  ego  can  be  de-formed,  can  and  does  break 
down  and  disintegrate  under  the  stress  of  situational  and  patho- 
logical conditions  (ch.  12).  This  formation,  re-formation,  and 
possible  de-formation  of  the  ego  completely  belie  any  notion  that 
the  ego  is  an  innate  entity. 


196  THE  GENETIC  FORMATION  OF  THE  EGO 


REFERENCES 

1.  BAIN,  R.,  The  self-and-other  words  of  a  child,  'Am.  J.  Social,  1936,  41,  767-75. 

2.  BALDWIN,  J.  M.,  Mental  Development  in  the  Child  and  the  Race,  New  York: 

Macmillan,  copyright  1895. 

3.  BEAVER,  A.  P.,  The  initiation  of  social  contacts  by  preschool  children:  A  study 

of  technique  in  recording  social  behavior,  Child.  Develop.  Monogr.,  Columbia 
Univ.:  Teachers  College,  1932, 7. 

4.  BERNE,  E.  V.  C.,  An  experimental  investigation  of  social  behavior  patterns  in 

young  children,  Univ.  Iowa  Stud. :  Stud,  in  Child  Welfare,  1930,  4,  no.  3. 

5.  BONNEY,  M.  E.,  A  study  of  social  status  on  the  second  grade  level,  /.  Genet. 

PsychoL,  1942,  60,  271-305. 

6.  BRIDGES,  K.  M.  B.,  The  Social  and  Emotional  Development  of  the  Pre-School 

Child,  London:  Kegan  Paul,  copyright  1931. 

7.  BUHLER,  C.,  The  social  behavior  of  the  child,  in  The  Handbook  °f  Child  Psy- 

chology (C.  Murchison,  ed.),  Worcester:  Clark  Univ.,  copyright  1933,  ch.  9. 

8.  From  Birth  to  Maturity,  London:  Kegan  Paul,  Trench,  Trubner,  copy- 
right 1935. 

9.  CHALLMAN,  R.  C.,  Factors  influencing  friendships  among  preschool  children, 

Child  Develop.,  1932,  3,  146-58. 
10.  CLARK,  K.  B.,  and  M.  K.  CLARK,  The  development  of  consciousness  of  self  and 

the  emergence  of  racial  identification  in  Negro  preschool  children,  /.  Soc. 

PsychoL,  1939,  10,  591-9. 
11. Skin  color  as  a  factor  in  racial  identification  of  Negro  preschool 

children,  /.  Soc.  PsychoL,  1940,  11,  159-69. 

12.  COOLEY,  C.  H.,  Human  Nature  and  the  Social  Order,  New  York:  Scribner's, 

1902. 

13.  A  study  of  the  early  use  of  self-words  by  a  child,  PsychoL  Rev.,  1908,  15, 

339-57. 

14.  DARWIN,  C.,  Biographical  sketch  of  an  infant,  Mind,  1877,  2,  285-94. 

15.  DAVIS,  A.,  Child  training  and  social  class,  in  Child  Behavior  and  Development 

(R.  G.  Barker,  J.  S.  Kounin,  H.  F.  Wright,  eds.),  New  York:  McGraw-Hill, 
copyright  1943,  ch.  34. 

16.  and  J.  DOLLARD,  Children  of  Bondage,  Washington:  Am.  Council  on 

Education,  1940. 

17.  FISHER,  M.  S.,  Language  Patterns  of  Preschool  Children,  New  York:  Columbia 

Univ.,  Teachers  College,  Bur.  Publications,  1934. 

18.  FURFEY,  P.  H.,  Some  factors  influencing  the  selection  of  boys'  chums,  /.  Appl. 

PsychoL,  1927,  11,  47-51. 

19.  GESELL,  A.,  et  aL,  The  First  Five  Years  of  Life,  New  York:  Harper,  copy- 

right 1940. 

20.  and  F.  L.  ILG,  Infant  and  Child  in  the  Culture  of  Today,  New  York: 

Harper,  copyright  1943. 

21. and  H.  THOMPSON,  Infant  Behavior,  New  York:  McGraw-Hill,  copyright 

1934. 

22. The  Psychology  of  Early  Growth,  New  York:  Macmillan,  1938. 

23.  GOODENOUGH,  F.  L.,  The  use  of  pronouns  by  young  children:  A  note  on  the 

development  of  self-awareness,  /.  Genet.  PsychoL,  1938,  52,  333-46. 


REFERENCES  ~    197 

24.  GOODENOUGH,  F.  L.,  Developmental  Psychology:  An  Introduction  to  the  Study 

of  Human  Behavior,  New  York:  Appletoa-Century,  copyright  1945. 

25.  GREEN,  E.  H.,  Group  play  and  quarreling  among  preschool  children,  Child 

Develop.,  1933,  4,  302-07. 

26.  HAGMAN,  E.  P.,  The  companionships  of  preschool  children,  Univ.  Iowa  Stud.: 

Stud,  in  Child  Welfare,  1933,  7,  no.  4. 

27.  HOROWITZ,  E.  L.,  Spatial  localization  of  the  self,  /.  Soc.  PsychoL,  1935,  6, 

379-87. 

28.  HOROWITZ,  R.  E.,  Racial  aspects  of  self-identification  in  nursery  school  children, 

/.  Psychol.,  1939,  7,  91-9. 

29. A  pictorial  method  for  study  of  self-identification  in  preschool  children, 

/.  Genet.  Psychol.,  1943,  62,  135-48. 

30.  HURLOCK,  E.  B.,  Child  Development,  New  York:  McGraw-Hill,  copyright 

1942. 

31.  ISAACS,  S.,  Intellectual  Growth  in  Young  Children,  London:  Routledge,  copy- 

right 1930. 

32. Social  Development  in  Young  Children,  London:  Routledge,  copyright 

J933. 

33.  JACK,  L.  M.,  An  experimental  study  of  ascendant  behavior  in  preschool  children, 

Univ.  Iowa  Stud.:  Stud,  in  Child  Welfare,  1934,  9,  no.  3. 

34.  JERSILD,  A.  T,,  Child  Psychology,  New  York:  Prentice-Hall,  copyright  1940. 

35.  and  M.  D.  FITE,  The  Influence  of  Nursery  School  Experience  on  Chil-. 

dren's  Social  Adjustments,  New  York:  Columbia  Univ.,  Teachers  College, 
Bur.  Publications,  1939. 

36.  KOCH,  H.  L.,  Popularity  in  preschool  children:  Some  related  factors  and  a 

technique  for  its  measurement,  Child  Develop.,  1933,  4,  164-75. 

37.  KOFFKA,  K.,  Principles  of  Gestalt  Psychology,  New  York:  Harcourt,  Brace, 

1935. 

38.  KOHLER,  W.,  Gestalt  Psychology,  New  York:  Liveright,  copyright  1929. 

39.  LEWIN,  K.,  Environmental  forces  in  child  behavior  and  development,  in  The 

Handbook^  of  Child  Psychology  (C.  Murchison,  ed.),  Worcester:  Clark  Univ., 
1931,  94-127. 
40. Dynamic  Theory  of  Personality,  New  York:  McGraw-Hill,  1935. 

41.  MACFARLANE,  J.  W.,  M.  P.  HONZIK,  M.  H.  DAVIS,  Reputation  differences  among 

young  school  children,  /.  Educ.  PsychoL,  1937,  28,  161-75. 

42.  MATEER,  R,  Child  Behavior,  Boston:  Gorham  Press,  1918. 

43.  MCCARTHY,  D.,  Language  development  of  the  preschool  child,  in  Child  Be- 

havior and  Development  (R.  G.  Barker,  J.  S.  Kounin,  H.  F.  Wright,  eds.), 
New  York:  McGraw-Hill,  1943,  ch.  7. 

44.  MEAD,  G.  H.,  Mind,  Self  and  Society,  Chicago:  Univ.  Chicago  Press,  1934. 

45.  MOORE,  K.  C.,  The  mental  development  of  a  child,  PsychoL  Rev.  Monogr. 

SuppL,  1896,  October,  1,  no.  3. 

46.  MURCHISON,  C.,  and  S.  LANGER,  Tiedemann's  observations  on  the  development 

of  the  mental  faculties  of  children,  /.  Genet.  PsychoL,  1927,  34,  205r30. 

47.  MURPHY,  L.  B.,  Social  Behavior  and  Child  Personality,  New  York:  Columbia 

Univ.,  copyright  1937. 

48.  PAGE,  M.  L.,  The  modification  of  ascendant  behavior  in  preschool  children, 

Univ,  Iowa  Stud.;  Stud,  in  Child  Welfare,  1936, 12,  no.  3. 


198  THE  GENETIC  FORMATION  OF  THE  EGO 

49.  PARTEN,  M.  B.,  Social  participation  among  preschool  children,  /.  'Abnorm.  & 

Soc.  Psychol.,  1932,  27,  243-69. 
50. Leadership  among  preschool  children,  /.  Abnorm.  &  Soc.  Psychol.,  1933, 

28,  430-40. 
51. Social  play  among  pre-school  children,  /.  Abnorm.  &  Soc.  Psychol. ,  1933, 

28,  136-47. 
52.  PIAGET,  J.,  Language  and  Thought  of  the  Child,  London:  Kegan  Paul,  1926. 

53. Judgment  and  Reasoning  in  the  Child,  New  York:  Harcourt,  Brace,  1928. 

54. Child's  Conception  of  Physical  Causality,  New  York:  Harcourt,  Brace, 

1930. 

55. Moral  Judgment  of  the  Child,  London:  Kegan  Paul,  1932. 

56. Children's  philosophies,  in  The  Handbook  of  Child  Psychology  (C. 

Murchison,  ed.),  Worcester:  Clark  Univ.,  1933,  ch.  12. 

57.  PREYER,  W.,  The  Mind  of  the  Child,  Part  II:  The  Development  of  the  Intellect, 

New  York:  Appleton,  copyright  1890. 

58.  SALUSXY,  A.  S.,  Collective  behavior  of  children  at  a  preschool  age,  /.  Soc. 

Psychol.  1930, 1,  367-78. 

59.  SHINN,  M.  W.,  Notes  on  the  Development  of  a  Child,  Berkeley:  Univ.  Press, 

Univ.  Calif.,  Publications  in  Education,  vol.  1,  1898;  vol.  2,  1907. 

60.  STERN,  W.,  Psychology  of  Early  Childhood,  New  York:  Holt,  copyright  1924. 

61.  STODDARD,  G.  D.,  and  B.  L.  WELLMAN,  Child  Psychology,  New  York:  Mac- 

millan,  1934. 

62.  SULLY,  J.,  Studies  of  Childhood,  New  York:  Appleton,  copyright  1895. 

63.  TRACY,  R,  and  J.  STIMPFL,  The  Psychology  of  Childhood,  Boston:  Heath, 

copyright  1909. 

64.  WALLON,  H.,  Les  Origines  du  caractere  chez  lf enfant,  Paris:  Presses  Universi- 

taires  de  France,  1933. 


CHAPTER  8 
RE-FORMATION  OF  THE  EGO  IN  ADOLESCENCE 

In  the  last  chapter  we  found  that  an  impressive  array  of  studies, 
accumulated  over  several  decades,  all  point  to  the  fact  that  the  ego 
is  a  genetic  formation,  developing  from  the  child's  contact  with 
his  physical  and  social  surroundings.  We  saw  what  diverse  fac- 
tors and  conditions  contribute  to  ego  formation.  The  infant 
learns,  for  example,  to  delineate  his  own  body  as  his;  he  learns 
that  he  has  a  name  and  around  this  name  or  personal  pronouns 
he  gathers  many  characteristics  that  define  his  psychological  iden- 
tity. With  the  acquisition  of  language,  ego-expansion  evolves  at 
a  more  rapid  pace,  encompassing  the  surrounding  standardized 
human  relationships  and  social  norms.  When  a  child  is  able  to 
grasp  reciprocal  relationships  extending  beyond  momentary  needs 
and  immediate  perceptual  reactions,  ego  development  is  affected 
in  an  important  way  by  his  absorbed  participation  in  age-mate 
groups. 

Yet  no  matter  how  important  these  factors  may  be,  even 
throughout  the  whole  life  of  an  individual,  any  account  of  ego 
development  as  it  functions  on  the  adult  level  will  suffer  major 
deficiencies  if  the  ego  problems  encountered  during  adolescence 
are  neglected.  For  there  are  certain  facts,  almost  truisms,  which 
make  the  period  of  genetic  development  from  early  to  late  ado- 
lescence crucial  for  the  psychology  of  ego  formation. 

THE  PROBLEM  OF  ADOLESCENCE  IN  A  SOCIOLOGICAL  AND 
PSYCHOLOGICAL  SETTING 

By  the  time  the  child  reaches  puberty,  he  has  become  a  mem- 
ber of  a  family,  a  social  class,  a  nation;  he  is  part  of  the  constella- 
tions formed  by  his  school,  by  his  neighborhood  friends,  or  by 
discriminations  based  on  sex.  All  of  these  situations  carry  their 

199 


200  RE-FORMATION  OP  THE  EGO  IN  ADOLESCENCE 

prevailing  values  which  determine  subsequent  attitudes.  But  the 
youngster  still  has  to  acquire  or  be  ascribed  a  full-fledged  adult 
male  or  female  status  with  its  particular  adult  rights,  privileges, 
and  responsibilities.  After  maturing  sexually  as  a  full-fledged 
male  or  female,  ready  to  function  physiologically,  almost  over- 
flowing, he  thus  faces  norms  and  serious  regulations  in  his  sur- 
roundings which  postpone,  deny,  or  regulate  his  sex  activities. 

Whatever  the  effects  of  infantile  sexuality  may  be  up  to  puberty 
(a  point  orthodox  psychoanalysts  have  been  so  concerned  about- 
sec  chapter  14)  a  whole  host  of  norms,  expectations,  and  regula- 
tions relating  to  femininity  and  masculinity  now  have  to  be  faced. 

In  some  societies,  the  individual  is  initiated  into  economic  life 
during  the  adolescent  period.  In  other  societies,  he  at  least  seri- 
ously starts  to  look  forward  or  to  prepare  for  some  trade  or  pro- 
fession. He  may  have  to  enter  into  the  economic  work  of  the 
adult  world  before  he  is  fully  matured  physiologically,  as  is  still 
the  case  in  countries  at  the  feudal  economic  level,  and  as  was  strik- 
ingly seen  during  the  period  of  ruthless  child  labor  in  the  last  few 
centuries.  Or  the  adolescent  may  have  to  assume  economic  re- 
sponsibilities owing  to  the  poverty  of  his  family.  Situations  such 
as  these  will  contribute  to  the  early  development  and  sharpening 
of  the  ego,  while  in  other  respects  the  youngster  may  lag  behind. 
This  is  likely  to  result  in  an  out-of-phase  development  which  pro- 
duces its  own  problems.  In  various  societies,  adolescents  may  be 
ascribed  a  definite  adult  status  in  earlier  or  later  adolescence.  And 
the  status  is,  of  course,  circumscribed  in  a  major  way  by  the  eco- 
nomic class  and  the  social  position  of  his  family. 

In  short,  the  years  covered  by  adolescence  are  usually  a  period 
of  transition  from  childhood  to  adult  status  with  all  its  economic, 
social,  and  sexual  aspects.  From  the  point  of  view  of  ego  develop- 
ment, a  period  during  which  status  changes  in  so  many  important 
aspects  of  life  should  be  of  crucial  significance.  For  shifts  in 
objective  status  are  reflected  as  ego-shifts  in  the  psychology  of  the 
individual.  Psychologically  speaking,  it  is  especially  in  the  ego 
of  the  individual  that  any  status  problem  finds  its  echo. 

When  societies  are  in  a  period  of  rapid  transition,  the  resulting 
confusions  in  status  create  added  problems  for  adolescents  who  are 
themselves  in  a  stage  of  transition  as  they  strive  for  adult  status. 


ADOLESCENCE  IN  DIFFERENT  CULTURES  AND  TIMES  201 

Today  almost  all  the  countries  of  the  world  are  in  such  a  state  of 
transition.  Even  the  patterns  of  culture  of  less  developed  peoples 
are  no  exception,  for  to  some  degree  they  are  affected  by  the 
inescapable  impact  of  the  technology  and  imperialisms  of  ad- 
vanced Western  societies.  In  addition,  the  contradictory  economic 
and  social  norms  which  antagonistic  groups  attempt  to  perpetuate 
contribute  their  share  in  making  status  problems  (ego  problems) 
more  complicated,  even  conflicting,  especially  for  adolescents. 
The  adolescent's  plight  is  still  further  complicated  by  the  alternat- 
ing treatment  he  receives  from  adults:  at  times  he  is  treated  as 
a  grown-up,  at  other  times  as  a  child. 

The  flux  and  crisis  situations  adolescents  have  to  go  through, 
at  times  unsuccessfully,  in  their  strivings  for  a  secure  social  status, 
provide  excellent  opportunity  for  the  study  of  many  problems 
concerned  with  the  psychology  of  the  ego  and  ego-involvements. 
Changes  brought  about  by  sexual  maturity,  by  significant  bodily 
developments  with  their  accompanying  serious  effects  on  attitudes 
of  masculinity  and  femininity,  shifts  (or  preparation  for  shifts) 
in  economic  roles,  and  actual  or  more  seriously  anticipated  shifts 
in  social  status,  all  make  the  period  of  adolescence  a  crucial  stage 
of  psychological  transition  involving  ego  problems.  We  have  de- 
liberately referred  to  this  further  development  of  the  ego  as  a 
period  of  re-formation.  In  view  of  the  complexity,  many-sided- 
ness, and  interrelatedness  of  the  problems  encountered  at  this 
stage,  they  should  be  considered  from  many  angles:  physiological, 
economic,  sociological,  and  others.  In  this  and  the  following  chap- 
ter, we  can  consider  only  those  features  of  adolescence  directly 
related  to  the  main  conceptual  scheme  of  this  book. 

ADOLESCENCE  IN  DIFFERENT  CULTURES  AND  TIMES 

In  dealing  with  problems  of  social  psychology,  there  is  almost 
always  the  danger  of  generalizing  on  the  basis  of  behavior  seen  in 
one  particular  culture.  Many  social  psychologists,  as  well  as  other 
social  scientists,  have  fallen  into  the  pitfalls  of  their  diverse  com- 
munity centrisms.  The  comparative  studies  of  different  cultures 
and  different  times  made  by  ethnologists  and  sociologists  help  us 
to  acquire  the  distance  necessary  to  relate  events  with  a  realistic 


202  RE-FORMATION  OF  THE  EGO  IN  ADOLESCENCE 

perspective.  For  example,  G.  Stanley  Hall's  [8]  colorful  drama- 
tizations of  adolescence  as  a  universal  period  of  "storm  and  stress" 
and  characterizations  of  primitive  societies  as  "adolescent"  are 
challenged  on  the  basis  of  comparative  observations.  The  pre- 
vailing romanticism  in  Europe  during  the  last  century  which 
made  for  the  immense  popularity  among  youth  of  books  like 
Werther  with  their  pessimistic  sentimentalism  is  almost  a  dead 
issue  today  for  the  descendants  of  these  very  youngsters.  The 
Wagnerian  heroics  and  ruthless  arrogance  displayed  in  no  uncer- 
tain terms  by  many  German  adolescents  who  under  the  well- 
organized  tutelage  of  Hitler-Jugend  even  spied  on  their  own  par- 
ents, seems  to  have  faded.  German  youth  today  presents  a  "pic- 
ture of  inconsistency  and  confusion."  [18,  4] 

Observations  found  in  diverse  studies  of  different  cultures  and 
times  lead  us  to  the  conclusion  that  the  special  problems  of  ado- 
lescence, the  ease  or  difficulty,  and  the  duration  of  youths'  transi- 
tion to  a  settled  adult  status  will  vary  with  the  economic  and 
social  circumstances  and  the  values  of  particular  cultures.  In  com- 
paratively simple  and  undifferentiated  societies,  the  transition  to 
adulthood  may  be  achieved  through  various  sorts  of  initiation 
ceremonies.  At  the  completion  of  these  ceremonies,  which  vary 
from  culture  to  culture  in  duration,  phase,  and  the  ordeals  en- 
tailed, the  young  person  may  find  himself  ascribed  with  adult 
status,  privileges,  and  responsibilities  with  their  unmistakable  and 
serious  psychological  consequences.  [19,  28,  30]  The  more  or  less 
stable  and  integrated  nature  of  social  organization  and  cultural 
norms  displayed  in  these  cultures  (more  so  formerly  than  now) 
are  important  factors  which  reduce  the  perplexities  and  confusions 
that  face  a  youth  in  his  transition  to  adulthood.  In  some  cultures, 
the  transition  to  adulthood  may  be  so  clearly  and  definitely  recog- 
nized that  the  boy  or  girl  may  be  designated  by  new  names  at 
these  initiation  ceremonies.  For  example,  the  Andamanese  girl 
acquires  a  "flower  name"  at  the  beginning  of  menstruation  [3, 
119]i  the  Arunta  boy  who  goes  through  the  laborious  initiation 
ceremonies  gets  different  names  at  different  phases  of  initiation. 
[24,  85  /.]  In  one  of  the  Melanesian  groups,  in  which  marriage 
takes  place  at  a  fairly  youthful  age,  both  parties  relinquish  their 
former  names  for  a  new  common  name.  [25,  347] 


ADOLESCENCE  IN  DIFFERENT  CULTURES  AND  TIMES  203 

Diverse  and  not  infrequently  contradictory  economic  and  social 
status  problems;  complicated  marriage  requirements,  considera- 
tions, and  norms  of  highly  differentiated  Western  societies  usually 
prolong  the  transition  stage  with  various  kinds  of  conflict  situa- 
tions and  suspensions.  In  these  societies,  therefore,  it  is  not  hard 
to  find  cases  of  restless  old  maids  or  eccentric  bachelors  who  dis- 
play fantasies  and  other  unsettled  modes  of  behavior  more  or  less 
peculiar  to  adolescents. 

A  glance  at  the  observations  of  the  adolescent  period  from  stud- 
ies of  primitive  cultures  reveals  diverse  types  of  adolescent  transi- 
tions. Some  transitions  are  rapid,  some  prolonged.  Some  are  full 
of  painful  ordeals;  others  are  comparatively  easy.  In  these  and 
other  respects,  different  gradations,  as  determined  by  the  economic 
and  cultural  characteristics  of  diverse  cultures,  can  easily  be  found 
in  the  welter  of  ethnological  literature.  The  social  and  cultural 
norms  regulating  the  activities  of  adolescent  boys  or  girls  may 
considerably  reduce  puzzling  adolescent  problems  and  difficulties. 
On  the  basis  of  Mead's  studies,  Klineberg  [11]  generalizes: 

Margaret  Mead  .  .  .  who  has  studied  Samoan  society  from  this 
point  of  view,  writes  that  there  the  adolescent  girl  differs  from  the 
non-adolescent  only  in  the  fact  of  bodily  changes;  there  is  no  conflict, 
no  revolt,  no  mental  disturbance  or  neurosis;  only  an  easy  transition 
to  a  new  status.  There  are  several  reasons  for  this,  as  Miss  Mead 
points  out.  The  problem  of  sex,  which  in  one  form  or  another  creates 
difficulties  for  almost  every  adolescent  in  our  society,  is  practically 
nonexistent.  Samoan  society  permits  premarital  intimacies,  and  shortly 
after  puberty  almost  every  young  boy  and  girl  enters  into  a  series  of 
"affairs."  These  can  hardly  be  called  "love  affairs,"  because  love  in 
the  romantic  sense  in  which  we  know  it  rarely  enters;  but  they  are 
for  that  reason  no  less  able  to  satisfy  an  urge  which  in  our  society  is 
probably  the  most  important  single  source  of  torment  and  disturbance. 
[11,  308] 

But  in  view  of  certain  facts  that  Margaret  Mead  herself  reports 
[20],  adolescent  girls  even  in  Samoa  seem  to  face  some  problems 
and  conflicting  situations,  unless  the  psychological  principles  that 
operate  in  them  are  altogether  different  from  those  of  other  human 
beings.  Thus: 


204  RE-FORMATION  OF  THE  EGO  IN  ADOLESCENCE 

The  first  attitude  which  a  little  girl  learns  towards  boys  is  one  of 
avoidance  and  antagonism.  .  .  .  After  a  little  girl  is  eight  or  nine  years 
of  age  she  has  learned  never  to  approach  a  group  of  older  boys.  [20, 
86] 

Again, 

Not  until  she  is  an  old  married  woman  with  several  children  will 
the  Samoan  girl  again  regard  the  opposite  sex  so  quietly.  [20,  86] 

Following  a  custom  of  Samoa,  the  young  man  sends  "a  confidant 
and  ambassador  whom  he  calls  a  soa"  to  secure  intimacy  with  a 
girl.  [89  f.] 

But  if  he  chooses  a  handsome  and  expert  wooer  who  knows  just 
how  "to  speak  softly  and  walk  gently,"  then  as  likely  as  not  the  girl 
will  prefer  the  second  to  the  principal.  This  difficulty  is  occasionally 
anticipated  by  employing  two  or  three  soas  and  setting  them  to  spy  on 
each  other.  But  such  a  lack  of  trust  is  likely  to  inspire  a  similar  atti- 
tude in  the  agents,  and  as  one  overcautious  and  disappointed  lover  told 
me  ruefully,  "I  had  five  soas,  one  was  true  and  four  were  false."  [20, 
90] 

Consequently, 

The  most  violent  antagonisms  in  the  young  people's  groups  are  not 
between  ex-lovers,  arise  not  from  the  venom  of  the  deserted  nor  the 
smarting  pride  of  the  jilted,  but  occur  between  the  boy  and  the  soa 
who  has  betrayed  him,  or  a  lover  and  the  friend  of  his  beloved  who 
has  in  any  way  blocked  his  suit.  [20,  97] 

It  would  be  surprising  if,  in  these  intrigues  in  sexual  affairs,  even 
though  they  may  be  of  short  duration,  the  girl,  too,  does  not  face 
some  complications.  Also,  as  Klineberg  [12]  later  indicated: 

Certain  problems  of  interpretation  in  connection  with  the  Samoan 
material  still  remain.  There  is,  for  instance,  the  fact  that  the  adoles- 
cent girls  do  not  engage  in  sex  activity  immediately  after  puberty; 
usually  there  is  an  interval  of  two  or  three  years.  [12,  493] 

In  view  of  some  other  observations  reported  by  Mead  to  the 
effect  that  the  wives  of  titled  men  derive  "their  status  from  their 
husbands"  [20,  77],  that  "a  girl's  chances  of  marriage  are  badly 
damaged  if  it  gets  about  the  village  that  she  is  lazy  and  inept  in 


ADOLESCENCE  IN  DIFFERENT  CULTURES  AND  TIMES  205 

domestic  tasks"  [33],  and  that  a  premium  is  put  on  virginity  for 
marriage  [98f.]>  it  seems  safe  to  say  that  girls  who  are  expected  to 
conform  to  these  norms  do  have  to  face  at  least  a  few  adolescent 
problems  in  their  transition  to  adulthood. 

In  other  studies  of  adolescence  in  primitive  societies,  Mead's  ma- 
terial indicates  that  this  period,  while  not  universally  a  period  of 
"storm  and  stress,"  does  involve  problems  varying  both  in  inten- 
sity and  kind  in  different  cultures,  depending  upon  their  socio- 
economic  structure  and  the  superstructure  of  norms  regulating 
behavior.  Another  society  in  which  the  period  might  be  consid- 
ered relatively  less  difficult  than  in  others  is  that  of  the  Arapesh 
of  New  Guinea  [22].  Among  them  certain  norms  deem  "that 
all  human  beings,  male  and  female,  are  naturally  unaggressive, 
self-denying,  lightly  sexed,  comfortably  domestic,  concerned  with 
growing  food  to  feed  growing  children."  [19,  xix}  In  spite  of 
the  fact,  as  Mead  reports  it,  that  children  of  seven  or  eight  have 
acquired  a  happy  and  confident  attitude  toward  life,  a  warm 
affection  toward  their  fellows,  and  respectful  unaggressive  treat- 
ment of  them,  the  adolescent  period  involves  the  formation  of 
new  attitudes  and  entrance  into  new  economic  and  social  rela- 
tionships. At  this  time,  Arapesh  children  are  for  the  first  time 
"made  culturally  self-conscious  of  the  physiology  of  sex."  [22,  62] 
The  parents  have  been  held  responsible  for  the  child's  growth 
until  then.  Now  the  boy  is  held  to  be  "the  responsible  custodian 
of  his  own  growth."  [62]  This  involves  taboos  relating  to  his 
genitals,  including  "disciplinary  and  hygienic  use  of  stinging  net- 
tles and  actual  bleeding  with  a  sharpened  bamboo  instrument." 
[62]  When  the  adolescent  boy  is  initiated,  he  gains  important 
secrets  of  the  tribe,  is  segregated  from  women,  observes  certain 
food  taboos,  and  is  incised. 

They  are  subjected  to  a  divinatory  ceremony  to  find  out  whether 
they  have  been  experimenting  with  sex  or  not,  something  that  they 
know  is  forbidden  because  it  will  stunt  natural  growth.  [22,  75] 

The  punishment  for  the  guilty  boy  involves  the  "deeply  felt"  hu- 
miliation of  publicly  violating  an  important  taboo.  The  boy  must 
also  stop  the  oral  play  in  which  he  has  engaged  since  childhood. 
On  the  other  hand,  certain  aspects  of  these  ceremonies  are  wholly 


206  RE-FORMATION  OF  THE  EGO  IN  ADOLESCENCE 

pleasant.  The  boys  are  well-fed  and  usually  emerge  "almost 
plump."  They  now  assume  many  of  their  fathers'  economic  and 
social  responsibilities. 

When  an  Arapesh  girl  is  seven  or  eight,  she  is  engaged  to  a  boy 
about  six  years  her  senior,  and  lives  in  his  family's  home.  Here 
she  lives  until  puberty  as  she  would  with  her  own  family.  As 
she  "approaches  puberty,  her  parents-in-law  increase  supervision 
of  her"  to  prevent  premarital  sex  activities.  An  elaborate  cere- 
mony occurs  at  the  girl's  first  menstruation,  much  of  the  ritual 
being  designed  "to  cut  out  the  girl's  connexion  with  her  past."  [93] 
She  fasts  for  five  or  six  days,  takes  part  in  public  ceremonies. 
However,  after  the  ceremonies,  "the  betrothed  girl's  life  goes  on 
as  before."  She  now  comes  to  think  of  her  betrothed  in  the  role 
of  a  future  husband  rather  than  in  the  big-brother  role  to  which 
she  has  become  accustomed.  There  is  a  period  of  waiting  before 
the  actual  marriage. 

Observations  on  the  Tchambuli  [22]  are  also  pertinent  for  our 
discussion  of  ego  problems  among  adolescents  of  diverse  cultures. 
For  among  them,  according  to  Mead,  there  is  a  variation  in  the 
intensity  and  kind  of  adolescent  problems  encountered  by  the 
two  sexes.  In  Mead's  words: 

The  Tchambuli  attempt  to  standardize  the  personality  of  the  sexes 
in  contrasting  ways — they  expect  men  to  be  responsive,  interested  in 
the  arts,  women  to  be  bold,  initiating,  economically  more  responsible. 
[19,**] 

The  situation  is  made  more  complicated  by  the  continuation  of 

.  .  .  patriarchal  forms  combined  with  personalities  more  appropriate 
to  matriarchy.  .  „  .  Such  mixed  and  badly  co-ordinated  elements 
cause  a  good  deal  of  confusion  and  functional  maladjustment,  espe- 
cially in  the  young  men.  [19,  xxi] 

Tchambuli  women  raise  the  food,  weave  the  mosquito  bags 
which  are  valuable  both  for  use  and  trade,  and  are  in  charge  of 
property,  although  men  are  the  nominal  owners.  Men,  on  the 
other  hand,  spend  their  time  in  art  work  and  preparing  for  elabo- 
rate ceremonials.  Until  just  before  adolescence,  the  boy  is  cared 
for  lavishly  by  women.  After  initiation  ceremonies, 


ADOLESCENCE  IN  DIFFERENT  CULTURES  AND  TIMES  207 

He  is  supposed  to  spend  more  time  in  the  men's  house,  but  he  still 
takes  refuge  among  the  women  whenever  possible.  He  grows  grad- 
ually into  young  manhood;  his  father  and  elder  brothers  watching 
jealously  his  attitude  towards  their  younger  wives  and  suspecting  him 
if  he  walks  about  upon  the  women's  road.  [22,  257] 

In  order  to  engage  in  sexual  activities,  he  must  be  chosen  by  a 
woman.  But  in  order  to  marry,  he  must  get  a  marriage  price 
from  his  male  relatives.  If  he  is  attractive,  versed  in  the  dance, 
soft-spoken,  and  resourceful,  he  may  be  chosen  by  more  than  one 
woman.  On  the  other  hand,  the  older  men  watch  the  boys  jeal- 
ously and  try  their  best  "to  shame  and  disgrace  them  before  the 
women."  [259] 

Although  Mead  gives  less  information  about  the  girls,  it  is  evi- 
dent that  adolescent  Tchambuli  girls  have  different  and  decidedly 
less  pressing  problems,  since  from  birth  they  grow  up  more  and 
more  a  part  of  the  solid  group  of  women,  whose  activities  are 
characterized  by  "comradeship,  efficient,  happy  work"  [252]  and 
comparatively  little  quarreling. 

In  contrast  to  the  Samoans  and  Arapesh,  the  Manus  adolescent 
faces  the  difficult  task  of  entering  an  adult  world  as  a  strikingly 
different  person  from  his  childhood  self.  The  Manus  of  the  Ad- 
miralty Islands  are  characterized  by  Mead  [21]  as 

.  .  .  driven  by  a  harsh  competitive  system,  hard  working  and  with 
little  tolerance  for  pleasure  or  art;  each  man  worked  for  himself  and 
for  his  own  household;  the  future  economic  security  of  one's  children 
was  a  principal  goal.  But  the  children  had  no  part  in  this  adult  world 
of  money  values  and  hard  work;  they  were  left  free  to  play  all  day 
in  a  pleasant  co-operative  world  where  there  was  no  property  and  no 
possessiveness.  .  .  .  And  yet,  when  they  passed  adolescence,  the  gener- 
ous gay  co-operative  Manus  children  turned  into  grasping  competitive 
Manus  adults.  [19,  xii] 

From  the  time  the  Manus  children  are  three,  they  are  made 
"ashamed  of  their  bodies,  ashamed  of  excretion,  ashamed  of  their 
sex  organs."  [21,  205]  When  a  girl  or  boy  is  engaged,  she  or  he 
can  no  longer  be  seen  in  the  presence  of  relatives  of  the  betrothed. 
The  breach  of  this  taboo  involves  as  much  shame  as  that  learned 
in  connection  with  the  body. 


208  RE-FORMATION  OF  THE  EGO  IN  ADOLESCENCE 

Puberty  for  girls  means  the  beginning  of  adult  life  and  responsibil- 
ity, the  end  of  play,  careless  companionship,  happy  hours  of  desultory 
ranging  through  the  village.  [21,  775] 

Because  of  the  taboos,  there  are  few  friends  available  to  die  ado- 
lescent girl  and  her  childhood  play  groups  break  up. 

She  makes  no  new  friends,  but  she  sees  less  and  less  of  her  old 
friends.  [775] 

At  menstruation  the  girl's  pact  with  her  sex  is  sealed  forever.  She 
learns  that  not  only  must  she  endure  first  menstruation,  but  the  strange 
fact,  the  fact  that  no  man  in  all  Manus  knows,  that  she  will  menstru- 
ate every  moon  and  must  hide  all  trace  or  knowledge  of  her  condition 
from  everyone.  [21,  757 /.] 

This  fact  "is  locked  away  in  the  girl's  mind  as  a  guilty  and  shame- 
ful secret."  [184]  For  several  months,  the  ceremonies  attending 
this  event  drag  on,  during  which  time  the  girl  must  stay  indoors. 

Past  puberty,  betrothed,  tabu,  and  respectable,  the  girl  is  expected  to 
settle  down  peacefully  to  her  labours,  to  submit  silently  to  eternal 
supervision.  The  slightest  breath  of  scandal  means  a  public  scene  and 
exaggerated  ignominy.  .  .  .  No  girl  can  manage  a  long  career  of 
rebellion.  While  she  sins,  all  of  her  kin,  her  betrothed's  kin,  her 
betrothed,  her  partner-in-sin,  she  herself,  are  in  danger  of  death  from 
the  ever  observant  spirits.  [21,  185] 

She  is  not  yet  a  full-fledged  member  of  society.    Ordinarily, 

These  years  are  not  years  of  storm  and  stress,  nor  are  they  years  of 
placid  unfolding  of  the  personality.  They  are  years  of  waiting,  years 
which  are  an  uninteresting  and  not  too  exacting  bridge  between  the 
free  play  of  childhood  and  the  obligations  of  marriage.  [21, 189] 

During  these  boring  years  as  a  "very  much  inhibited  spectator," 
"she  gets  the  culture  by  heart."  [189]  Some  girls  rebel. 

When  Manus  boys  reach  puberty,  their  gay  play  is  interrupted 
for  a  few  weeks  to  have  their  ears  pierced  and  to  take  part  in  a' 
feast.  Then  they  return  to  their  play.  However,  by  this  time,  the 
girls  of  their  age  have  retired,  and  the  play  group  is  composed  onlv 
of  younger  children  and  male  age  mates. 

The  boys  form  closer  friendships,  go  about  more  in  pairs,  make 
more  of  the  casual  homosexuality  current  in  childhood.  There  is 


ADOLESCENCE  IN  DIFFERENT  CULTURES  AND  TIMES  209 

much  roughhouse,  arm  linking,  whispered  conferences,  sharing  of 
secret  caches  of  tobacco.  [21, 193] 

Because  of  this  absorption  in  his  age-mate  group,  the  boy  rebels 
actively  if  his  parents  decide  upon  his  initiation  before  most  of  his 
age  mates  have  theirs. 

A  year  or  so  later,  "all  Manus  boys  go  away  to  work— two  years, 
five  years,  sometimes  seven  years — for  the  white  man."  [196] 
During  this  time,  the  boy  may  be  "lonely  and  homesick,  over- 
worked, hungry,  sulky,  shrinking  and  afraid,"  or  he  may  have 
strange  and  absorbing  new  experiences  and  friendships.  However, 
in  either  case,  his  experiences  in  no  way  prepare  him  for  adult 
life  in  his  village,  "of  which  he  has  a  fundamental  dread."  [200] 

"Now  comes  the  time  when  the  young  man  must  marry."  [206] 
Since  he  is  in  no  way  prepared,  he  must  plunge  heavily  into  debt 
for  household  and  work  equipment  which  no  Manus  boy  pos- 
sesses. His  life  undergoes  a  radical  change.  He  becomes  utterly 
shamefacedly  dependent  on  his  backers.  Before  him  lies  the  op- 
portunity of  regaining  self-respect  in  his  society  by  "hard  dealing, 
close-fisted  methods,  stinginess,  saving,  ruthlessness"  or  continued 
dependence  and  shame.  [208  /.] 

If  ...  childhood  had  never  been,  if  every  father  had  set  about 
making  his  newborn  son  into  a  sober,  anxious,  calculating,  bad  tem- 
pered little  businessman,  he  could  hardl"  have  succeeded  more  per- 
fectly. [21,  210] 

In  another  tribe  studied  by  Mead  [22],  the  Mundugumors,  ado- 
lescence is  so  frequently  a  period  of  intensified  struggle  and  pas- 
sion, that  we  shall  mention  only  a  few  of  the  problems  met  there. 

They  [the  Mundugumors]  assume  that  all  children,  male  and 
female,  are  naturally  aggressive  and  hostile.  [19,  xx] 

As  a  result  of  a  Spartan-like  childhood, 

Pre-adolescent  Mundugumor  children  have  an  appearance  of  harsh 
maturity  and,  aside  from  sex-experience,  are  virtually  assimilated  to 
the  individualistic  patterns  of  their  society  by  the  time  they  are  twelve 
or  thirteen.  Initiation  comes  to  girls  as  somewhat  of  a  privilege 
granted  to  them  in  proportion  as  they  are  aggressive  and  demanding, 
to  boys  as  a  penalty  they  cannot  escape.  .  .  .  [22,  212] 


210  RE-FORMATION  OF  THE  EGO  IN  ADOLESCENCE 

Under  such  circumstances,  the  acquisition  of  adult  status  could 
hardly  be  expected  to  be  a  pleasant  process  for  the  Mundugumor 
adolescent. 

There  is  a  premium  upon  virginity,  and  a  vigorous,  positively  sexed 
group  of  young  girls  who  plan  their  own  affairs  in  spite  of  a  restrictive 
chaperonage.  There  is  a  social  standard  which  prescribes  that  the 
sister  is  used  in  payment  for  her  brother's  wife,  and  a  continuous 
flouting  of  this  standard  by  her  father,  her  brother,  and  the  sisterless 
lover  who  attempts  to  abduct  her.  [22,  222  f.] 

As  a  result  of  these  practices,  the  adolescent  girl  or  boy  may  find 
himself  married  to  a  boy  or  girl  literally  years  younger  than  he 
or  she.  Even  after  marriage,  the  struggle  to  satisfy  basic  needs 
and  to  gain  satisfactory  adult  status  continues. 

In  his  account  of  the  adolescent  period  of  the  Kwomas  of  New 
Guinea,  Whiting  writes: 

Next  to  the  weaning  period,  adolescence  is  the  most  turbulent  time 
in  the  life  of  a  Kwoma  individual,  a  period  marked  by  the  learning  of 
many  new  habits  and  the  facing  of  many  new  problems.  [31,  65] 

Kwoma  boys  enter  adolescence  formally  with  their  initiation  into 
the  first  age-grade  group,  whereas  girls  undergo  rites  at  the  time 
of  their  first  menstruation.  The  period  is  closed  formally  at  a 
ceremony  in  which  the  boy  or  girl  is  cicatrized,  at  about  the  age 
of  sixteen  or  seventeen.  The  ceremonies  for  adolescent  boys  and 
girls  include  a  period  of  seclusion  for  about  two  months,  while 
the  boys  undergo  painful  trials,  dancing,  and  other  tests.  On  this 
occasion,  sexual  license  is  permitted  to  all  except  the  youngest 
initiates  (pubescent  boys).  Since  the  age-grade  ceremony  for  the 
boys  occurs  only  once  every  five  years,  the  formal  period  of  ado- 
lescence for  boys  may  last  from  one  to  three  years  and  may  cor- 
respond only  roughly  to  his  pubescent  period.  Because  these  rites 
give  a  boy  "privileges  and  immunities  which  Kwoma  culture 
grants  to  persons  in  this  category"  [67],  the  disparity  between 
physiological  development  and  status  in  the  group  resulting  from 
the  infrequency  of  the  ceremonies  would  presumably  be  trying 
for  the  boy  who  develops  between  ceremonies  or  shortly  after  a 
ceremony.  The  initiation  does,  in  fact,  give  the  adolescent  "a  de- 


ADOLESCENCE  IN  DIFFERENT  CULTURES  AND  TIMES  211 

fined  status  superior  to  those  of  his  brothers  who  have  not  yet 
undergone  them.  Whereas  during  childhood  a  command-obey 
relationship  had  to  be  fought  out,  and  was  usually  established  on 
the  basis  of  size  and  age,  now  the  relationship  is  culturally  de- 
termined." [92] 

Economically,  the  boy  or  girl  now  becomes  an  asset,  engaging 
in  tasks  prescribed  for  members  of  his  sex.  The  adequate  per- 
formance of  these  new  tasks  has  a  direct  bearing  on  the  status  of 
the  adolescent,  for 

One  of  the  criteria  by  which  a  girl  chooses  a  man  for  a  lover  or  a 
husband  is  his  ability  as  a  worker.  Other  things  equal,  the  more 
industrious  a  boy  is,  the  more  he  is  sought  after  by  the  girls.  Lazy 
lads  are  shunned.  .  .  .  Industriousness  on  the  part  of  the  girl  is  an 
even  more  important  criterion  of  attractiveness  than  for  a  boy.  [72] 

However,  full  participation  in  the  economic  and  social  life  is  not 
yet  possible.  Restrictions  are  put  on  such  acts  as  the  performance 
of  certain  tasks,  on  approaches  to  members  of  the  opposite  sex,  ex- 
posure of  certain  parts  of  the  body.  Philandering  before  marriage 
does  occur,  but  it  is  frequently  accompanied  by  anxiety  since  the 
boy  faces  danger  from  the  girl's  relatives,  and  the  girl  is  under 
pressure  from  her  brothers  who  fear  that  if  her  reputation  is  poor 
"her  value  as  a  potential  wife"  will  be  decreased. 

The  Kwoma  adolescent  suffers  certain  contradictions  in  status 
since,  at  this  time,  when  he  thinks  of  himself  and  fulfills  the  activi- 
ties of  a  near-adult,  he  must  continue  "to  behave  towards  his  par- 
ents and  his  paternal  uncles  and  aunts  as  he  did  in  childhood." 
Sometimes  the  adolescent  may  revolt  against  this  continued  au- 
thority of  elders.  An  adolescent  boy  may  also  retaliate  against 
what  he  feels  as  unjust  treatment  by  breaking  or  threatening  to 
break  "his  relationship  with  any  adult  or  adolescent  relative."  [94] 

As  another  cultural  variation  of  adolescent  problems,  we  sum- 
marize briefly  a  few  points  from  the  case  of  an  American  Indian. 
The  anthropologist  Paul  Radin  secured  and  published  the  auto- 
biography of  a  Winnebago  Indian  [23].  This  intimate  life  history, 
presented  in  longitudinal  sequence,  is  especially  valuable  in  re- 
vealing the  reciprocal  effects  of  the  impact  of  culture  and  the 
counteracting  adolescent  impulses.  The  Winnebago  adolescent  is 


212  RE-FORMATION  OF  THE  EGO  IN  ADOLESCENCE 

required  to  fast  in  seclusion  for  days  so  that  he  may  be  blessed  by 
the  spirits.  We  see  in  the  autobiography  how  conflicts  between 
cultural  norms  and  the  urge  for  eating  take  place  under  the  stress 
of  hunger  and  how  the  dictates  of  hunger  prevail.  Likewise,  a 
psychological  situation  arising  from  a  Winnebago  sexual  taboo  is 
interesting.  Among  the  Winnebago, 

Any  contact  with  menstruating  women,  or  even  with  objects  in  any 
way  connected  with  them,  will,  it  is  believed,  destroy  the  power  of 
sacred  objects  or  individuals  temporarily  sacred.  [23,  387] 

Hence  menstruating  girls  are  secluded  in  lodges.  This  particular 
Winnebago  Indian  gives  detailed  accounts  of  how  he  and  other 
boys  took  advantage  of  this  seclusion  of  girls  to  approach  them 
and  of  the  careful  planning  and  hardships  they  had  to  go  through 
to  enter  the  lodges  secretly  at  night. 

In  a  memorable  chapter  on  "The  Crises  of  Life  and  Transition 
Rites,"  Radin  [24],  who  is  more  concerned  with  the  recurrent 
human  and  socioeconomic  implications  of  his  material  than  with 
exotic  presentations,  gives  a  penetrating  synthesis  of  these  transition 
rites. 

Two  distinct  sets  of  circumstances,  one  physiological,  the  other  eco- 
nomic-social, thus  conspired  to  make  of  puberty  one  outstanding 
focus  which  was  to  serve  as  the  prototype  for  all  other  periods  inter- 
preted as  transitional.  [24,  79] 

In  short,  these  rites  mark  definite  shifts  in  the  social  and  economic 
status  of  the  individual  and  the  "passing  of  an  individual  from 
the  position  of  an  economic  liability  to  that  of  an  economic  and 
social  asset."  [79]  This  new  status  generates  new  ego-attitudes 
related  to  work,  to  the  opposite  sex,  and  to  other  persons.  Taking 
as  his  examples  the  Arunta  of  Australia,  the  Selknam  of  Tierra 
del  Fuego,  the  New  Caledonians,  the  Ashanti  of  West  Africa,  and 
the  Thonga  of  South  Africa,  Radin  shows  variations  in  transition 
as  determined  by  the  socioeconomic  organization  and  the  deriva- 
tive cultural  norms  existing  in  each  of  these  cultures.  The  relative 
ease  or  difficulty  and  duration  of  the  transition  in  each  case  is 
especially  dependent  on  the  particular  socioeconomic  circumstances 
of  society.  To  cite  another  variation  from  Radin's  material,  among 


ADOLESCENCE  IN  DIFFERENT  CULTURES  AND  TIMES  213 

the  Ashanti  of  West  Africa  (unlike  many  other  cultures),  "only 
the  girls  go  through  a  puberty  ceremony."  [94]  The  girls  are 
"carried  at  certain  parts  of  the  ritual"  because  at  this  period  "they 
are  newly  born  and  cannot  walk."  [94] 

Radin  maintains  that,  in  spite  of  all  of  the  variations  displayed 
as  steps  of  transition  from  adolescence  to  adulthood,  their  basic 
function,  the  "basic  formula,"  is  the  same  in  all  of  them,  that  is, 
rendering  the  child  "an  economic  and  social  asset"  from  a  state  of 
dependence  and  liability.  The  existence  of  puberty  rites  for  girls 
alone  among  the  Ashanti  of  West  Africa  is  no  exception  to  the 
basic  formula. 

In  the  case  of  women  the  physiological  facts — the  first  menstruation, 
pregnancy,  and  childbirth — at  first  dwarfed  the  social-economic 
factors;  in  the  case  of  men  the  social-economic  factors  from  the  very 
beginning  dwarfed  the  physiological.  This  was  natural  enough  at  a 
period  in  man's  existence  where  woman's  position  was  at  best  undif- 
ferentiated,  politically  and  economically.  The  social-economic  im- 
plications of  this  situation  thus  being  so  much  more  important  for  the 
man,  it  is  not  surprising  that  the  puberty  rites  for  him  have  always 
remained  far  more  complex  and  differentiated  than  those  for  the 
woman.  The  latter  became  progressively  more  complex  as  her  eco- 
nomic functions  became  more  important,  after  the  introduction  of 
agriculture,  for  instance.  Occasionally,  as  in  some  West  African 
tribes,  puberty  rites  exist  for  her  only.  [24,  83] 

Psychologically,  all  of  these  steps,  trials— ceremonies  and  preach- 
ings—which achieve  the  transition  of  the  adolescent  to  the  adult 
status  mean  formation  of  attitudes  related  to  his  new  relative  role 
in  society,  his  conformity  in  respecting  the  property  and  sex  rights 
of  elders  and  interest  groups,  his  settling  down  in  the  place  as- 
signed him  by  the  established  authority  of  his  society. 

But  we  do  not  even  have  to  go  to  Western  Africa  or  the  distant 
Pacific  islands  to  achieve  perspective.  The  social  psychologist  can 
also  acquire  it  if  he  has  won  a  historical  sense  with  respect  to  his 
culture.  Here  is  a  concrete  illustration  of  this  lack  of  perspective. 
G.  Stanley  Hall  in  1904  devoted  a  chapter  of  82  pages  to  the  prob- 
lems of  religious  conversions  connected  with  adolescence.  [8,  vol. 
2,  ch.  14]  With  respect  to  religious  conversions,  Hall  states,  for 
instance: 


^H  RE-FORMATION  OF  THE  EGO  IN  ADOLESCENCE 

In  its  most  fundamental  sense,  conversion  is  a  natural,  normal, 
universal,  and  necessary  process  at  the  stage  when  life  pivots  over 
from  an  autocentric  to  an  heterocentric  basis.  [8,  vol.  2,  301] 

Four  decades  later,  in  the  same  country,  Harold  Jones  [10],  one  of 
the  outstanding  authorities  in  the  field  of  adolescence,  has  to 
correct  in  more  modest  terms  what  Hall  had  held  to  be  a  universal 
fact. 

As  a  symptom  of  adolescence,  religious  "conversion"  is  probably 
less  common  now  than  in  the  generation  of  which  G.  Stanley  Hall 
wrote.  Sometimes,  indeed  we  note  the  opposite  of  conversion,  in  an 
adolescent  revolt  which  leads  to  an  active  repudiation  of  religious 
concepts  and  practices.  [10,  108] 

This  trend  is  substantiated  by  others.  For  instance,  a  "Boy  Survey" 
by  Middletown's  Optimist  Club  of  3,771  boys  in  the  county  indi- 
cated that  only  39  per  cent  of  boys  from  8  to  16  years  of  age  were 
church  members.  [14,  304]  A  more  recent  substantiation  comes 
in  a  survey  made  by  Weaver  [29],  himself  a  professor  of  religion. 
Weaver  asked  high  school  principals,  judges,  chairmen  of  church 
and  school  boards,  and  active  laymen  "in  many  urban  centers"  to 
guess  "what  percentage  of  boys  and  girls  in  your  community  14-18 
years  of  age  have  an  appreciable  relationship  with  the  church, 
church  school,  or  church  young  people's  societies  ?  By  'appreciable' 
I  mean  something  less  than  regular  attendance  but  something 
more  than  'just  Easter  and  Christmas.'"  The  guesses  ranged 
"from  15  per  cent  to  30  per  cent  of  the  youth  population."  [159] 
On  the  basis  of  this  and  other  surveys  he  reviewed,  Weaver  con- 
cludes: 

Youth  are  less  interested  and  less  active  in  organized  religion  than 
in  any  other  major  institution  of  our  culture.  [29,  756] 

Until  recently,  it  was  almost  customary,  especially  in  elite  circles, 
to  think  of  young  Russian  intellectuals  as  highly  introspective  and 
pessimistic,  something  like  the  Dostoievsky  character,  Roskolnikov. 
But  in  reality,  the  behavior  of  the  young  Comsomols  as  observed, 
for  example,  by  Frankwood  Williams  [32]  even  in  the  early  1930's, 
and  the  majority  of  artistic  expressions  of  Soviet  novels  and  films, 
such  as  The  Road  to  Ufa  the  New  Gulliver,  Zoya,  and  Simonov's 


ADOLESCENCE  IN  DIFFERENT  CULTURES  AND  TIMES  215 

Days  and  Nights,  reflect  an  abounding  optimism  and  good-natured 
striving.  In  fact,  the  Russian  generations  brought  up  under  the 
Soviet  system  reflect  characteristics  which  are  almost  the  exact 
opposite  of  morbid  introspection,  self-pity,  or  self-justification. 

In  a  highly  developed,  highly  differentiated,  and  changing  social 
system  in  which  cultural  norms  of  past  centuries  exist  side  by  side 
with  new  norms  brought  about  chiefly  by  technological  develop- 
ments, adolescents  face  conflicting  values  which  make  their  transi- 
tion to  settled  adult  life  more  difficult.  In  their  penetrating  analysis 
of  Middletown  in  Transition,  the  Lynds  write: 

Today,  in  the  presence  of  such  rigorous  tenacity  to  the  "old,  tried 
ways"  by  part  of  the  population,  the  range  of  sanctioned  choices  con- 
fronting Middletown  youth  is  wider,  the  definition  of  the  one  "right 
way"  less  clear.  That  this  is  the  normal  situation  in  the  process  we 
call  "social  change"  does  not  lessen  the  confusion  it  entails.  ...  If  the 
child  up  to  high-school  age  associates,  by  reason  of  the  assignment  of 
each  child  to  a  grade  school  on  the  basis  of  residential  propinquity, 
with  other  children  from  somewhat  similar  subcultural  backgrounds, 
this  homogeneity  of  sorts  is  lost  when  the  children  pour  from  all 
quarters  into  Central  High  School.  Here  the  whole  range  of  cultural 
tolerances  and  intolerances  grind  against  each  other;  the  child  of 
parents  who  think  it  "cute"  and  "attractive"  for  a  daughter  to  enamel 
her  nails,  use  rouge,  have  a  crisp  "permanent,"  and  "learn  to  handle 
boys"  sits  next  to  the  daughter  of  a  family  in  which  the  parents  are 
engaged  in  a  quiet  but  determined  campaign  to  circumvent  the  influ- 
ence of  the  movies  and  to  keep  their  daughter  "simple,"  "unaffected," 
and  "healthy-minded."  This  widening  of  contacts  with  unevenly 
sanctioned  choices,  supported  not  by  outlaw  individuals  but  by  groups, 
means  under  these  circumstances  for  both  parents  and  children  un- 
certainty and  tension.  [14,  775] 

To  get  out  of  these  conflicting  situations,  in  some  cases,  young 
people  in  high  school  may  try  to  find  security  in  early  marriages. 

It  is,  as  noted  above,  a  less  uncommon  occurrence  in  these  1930's 
than  it  was  ten  years  ago  for  high  school  students  to  marry  and  thus 
to  seek  to  hew  a  path  of  freedom  out  of  the  cultural  conflicts,  un- 
certainties, and  stubborn  parental  restraints  in  which  they  find  them- 
selves. [14,  17 5  f.] 

In  other  cases,  the  difficult  economic  situations  delay  marriages. 
As  the  Lynds  note,  during  the  depression  in  Middletown,  young 


216  RE-FORMATION  OF  THE  EGO  IN  ADOLESCENCE 

people  just  had  to  "stick  around/'  while  "the  repetitiveness  of  this 
familiar  round  of  life  among  neighbors  may  become  acutely  dis- 
tasteful if  either  of  man's  great  peacetime  anodynes  for  routine — 
marriage  and  work— are  denied  him."  [486]  This  was  the  pre- 
dicament of  "Middletown  youngsters."  Marriage  often  had  to  be 
delayed  because  there  were  no  jobs. 

As  these  observations  show,  a  major  economic  event  such  as  a 
depression  which  further  sharpens  economic  differences  between 
the  classes  may  be  reflected  in  a  sharpening  of  the  differences 
among  adolescent  groups.  These  conflicts  no  doubt  have  psycho- 
logical consequences.  Turning  again  to  Middletown,  for  example, 
the  Lynds  note  that  the  standards  for  dress  in  the  high  school 
groups  were  set  by  the  more  well-to-do  North  Side  girls.  The 
depression  meant  that  the  poorer  girls  from  the  South  Side  were 
considerably  less  up  to  standard  than  before.  These  girls  were 
very  distressed,  "some  of  them  to  the  point  of  withdrawing  from 
high-school."  [445]  The  status  of  these  South  Side  students  was 
further  handicapped  "by  the  presence  among  their  number  of  an 
increasing  number  of  marginal  persons  who  are  going  on  into 
high  school  because  they  cannot  get  jobs."  [452]  One  result  of 
this  stress  on  the  South  Side  adolescent  was  that  "some  boys  and 
girls  now  no  longer  dare  brave  the  front  door  of  the  high  school 
'with  the  steps  crowded  with  richer  students  looking  you  over,' 
but  go  around  to  the  side  entrance."  [452] 

The  depression  seems  to  have  brought  about  particularly  severe 
social  problems  for  girls.  For  even  if  a  girl  did  not  have  to  assume 
the  economic  responsibility  for  the  family  or  engage  in  serious 
forms  of  deviant  behavior,  she  may  have  had  to  quit  school  at  an 
early  age  "because  of  runs  in  her  stockings,  torn  shoes,  and  worn- 
out  dresses."  [13,  95]  Or,  she  might  become  "afraid  to  bring  any 
friends  of  hers  to  her  home"  and  have  to  evade  their  invitations  to 
go  out  after  school  hours  because  she  has  no  money.  "Her  girl 
friends  don't  understand,  and  she  is  afraid  she  is  losing  their 
friendship."  [96] 

A  special  study  of  "Youth  in  the  Depression"  made  by  Kingsley 
Davis  [4],  gives  a  realistic  picture  of  the  way  a  major  economic 
event  can  further  aggravate  the  feelings  of  insecurity  in  a  great 
number  of  young  people. 


ADOLESCENCE  IN  DIFFERENT  CULTURES  AND  TIMES  217 

Boys  and  girls  all  over  the  country  find  nothing  but  a  blank  wall 
in  front  of  them.  With  their  parents  suffering,  their  own  futures 
clouded,  the  ordinary  roads  to  success  closed,  they  blindly  seek  a  way 
out.  In  such  conditions  youth  movements  spring  up.  [4,  12] 

If  not  caught  in  actual  youth  movements  of  various  kinds,  young 
boys  or  girls  at  least  spontaneously  develop  more  or  less  secret  or 
confidential  groups,  cliques  or  gangs,  among  themselves.  From 
these  they  derive  a  certain  satisfaction  of  status  and  security  in  a 
contradictory,  apparently  hostile,  social  world. 

Extraordinary  changes  in  the  behavior  of  adolescents  are  also, 
of  course,  brought  about  by  war.  We  already  have  some  sig- 
nificant studies  indicating  the  impact  of  World  War  II  on  adoles- 
cents in  the  United  States,  a  country,  we  should  recall,  that  was 
comparatively  little  affected  by  the  war.  Not  only  did  thousands 
of  late  adolescents  have  "maturity  thrust  upon  them"  by  entrance 
into  the  armed  services  [6,  62],  but  in  the  United  States  alone  the 
adolescent  labor  force  from  ages  14  to  18  grew  from  872,314  in 
1940  to  3,000,000  in  1944,  with  others  working  part-time  and  dur- 
ing vacations,  [15]  While  many  quickly  became  "adult,"  many 
others  suffered  from  this  aggravated  contradiction  in  status.  [1,  7, 
33]  Adolescent  workers  in  war  plants,  for  example,  were  some- 
times only  "tolerated"  by  adult  workers,  by  adults  in  the  commu- 
nities, and  by  parents.  [7,  27,  26]  In  addition  to  the  greater  dis- 
parity between  the  values  of  adolescents  and  parents  created  by 
the  acceleration  of  cultural  change  brought  about  by  the  war,  such 
conflicting  situations  resulted  in  increased  parent-youth  conflict, 
increased  drifting  away  from  home  and  grown-ups.  [2,  39]  In 
particular,  the  war  intensified  the  problems  of  the  younger  adoles- 
cents. Although  feeling  "die  restlessness,  excitement,  and  anxiety 
that  war  brings,"  they  found  little  opportunity  to  share  in  it  all  and 
were  often  held  back  by  their  parents.  [38]  One  result  of  these 
conditions  for  young  adolescent  girls,  who  take  the  "next  older 
group"  or  "those  just  older"  as  their  reference  group,  was  the  great 
rise  in  sex  delinquency.  [6,  9]  This  was  found  even  among  girls 
"from  families  that  are  of  high  quality."  [7,  30]  Additional 
problems  came  to  those  adolescents  whose  homes  were  broken  by 
the  war,  whose  families  migrated  to  strange  communities,  or 
whose  parents  worked  away  from  home  during  wartime.  [2,  26] 


218  RE-FORMATION  OF  THE  EGO  IN  ADOLESCENCE 

Davis  [5]  has  given  a  cogent  sociological  characterization  of  the 
main  problems  relating  to  "adolescence  and  the  social  structure." 
He  emphasizes  the  point  that,  as  a  result  of  the  conflicting  norms 
relating  to  such  matters  as  maturity  status,  profession,  marriage, 
in  a  society  of  rapid  social  change,  the  problems  adolescent  boys 
and  girls  face  "involve  strain  and  inconsistency,"  making  the 
transition  more  or  less  an  ordeal,  prolonging  it  to  a  greater  or 
lesser  degree,  and  in  some  cases  creating  inevitable  parent-youth 
conflict.  In  a  separate  study,  Davis  takes  up  specifically  "The 
Sociology  of  Parent-Youth  Conflict"  (pp.  287/.). 

Davis  further  indicates  the  specific  social  inconsistencies  and 
contradictions  of  occupational  placement,  of  the  institutions  and 
norms  regulating  sexual  behavior,  and  of  the  rights  and  privileges 
of  adolescents.  Calling  attention  to  such  contradictions,  he  writes: 

In  our  society,  even  apart  from  the  family,  the  adolescent  finds  an 
absence  of  definitely  recognized,  consistent  patterns  of  authority. 
Because  of  the  compartmentalization  of  the  culture  he  is  defined  at 
times  as  an  adult,  at  other  times  as  a  child.  [5,  I?/ italics  ours] 

In  contrast  to  such  casual  and  contradictory  patterns,  Davis  points 
out  how,  in  a  different  society,  the  Soviet  Union,  adolescents  are 
an  organic  part  of  an  integrated  social  system;  how,  for  example, 
the  youth  in  school  and  in  the  Pioneer  and  Comsomol  organiza- 
tions play  a  responsible  role  in  harmony  with  their  maturity  level. 
On  the  basis  of  such  considerations,  he  concludes: 

The  Soviet  system  suggests  that  to  make  the  school  an  integral  part 
of  the  political  and  economic  structure,  and  to  give  youth  a  produc- 
tive role,  central  planning  of  the  whole  economy  is  necessary.  [5,  15] 

A  study  such  as  this  forcefully  brings  home  once  more  the  futility 
of  any  study  of  the  social  psychology,  or  of  any  psychology  for 
that  matter,  of  adolescence  without  first  placing  the  individual  in 
his  socioeconomic  setting.  In  a  later  chapter  we  include  a  few 
specific  illustrations  of  individual  adolescent  behavior  in  group 
situations  which  are  in  harmony  with  the  general  trend  of  society 
at  large  (pp.  338/.). 

The  full  importance  of  a  knowledge  of  the  setting  within  which 
the  adolescent  develops  can  be  seen  if  we  realize  that,  under  certain 


ADOLESCENCE  IN  DIFFERENT  CULTURES  AND  TIMES  219 

conditions,  even  schooling  may  be  a  curse  instead  of  a  blessing  for 
youths  in  the  process  of  becoming  grown-ups.  A  concrete  illus- 
tration of  the  point  is  reported  by  Majumdar  [16]  in  his  study  of 
"A  Tribe  in  Transition/'  the  Ho  tribe  of  the  Ghota  plateau  in 
India.  Among  other  interesting  problems  facing  this  colonial 
tribe,  he  reports  the  effects  of  schools  introduced  by  the  British 
government.  Education  in  the  local  schools  "does  not  help  the 
Hos  in  the  least  from  an  economic  point  of  view,  unless  the  stu- 
dents can  proceed  to  the  High  School  at  Chaibassa."  [197]  Even 
then  the  student  will  probably  not  receive  a  remunerative  job  from 
the  British  government.  However,  children  of  the  landowners 
can  return  to  be  "the  leaders  in  the  village."  The  usual  reaction 
of  the  average  Hos  who  go  to  high  school  is  to  "return  to  their 
homes  and  curse  their  education  for  the  rest  of  their  lives"  after 
two  or  three  years  of  moving  "heaven  and  earth  to  secure  a 
job."  [198] 

As  we  shall  see,  adolescents  frequently  find  relief  from  puzzling 
inconsistent,  confusing  situations,  by  "rebelling"  against  the  dic- 
tates of  adults  in  their  immediate  environment  and  anchoring 
themselves  in  an  age-mate  world,  or,  specifically,  in  age-mate 
groups  (pp.  251  $.).  Their  behavior  is  then  regulated  in  an  impor- 
tant way  by  the  values  or  norms  prevalent  among  the  age  mates 
to  whom  they  refer  themselves.  Many  of  these  norms  may  run 
counter  to  those  of  their  parents  and  other  adults.  However,  this 
picture,  if  not  completed,  leaves  an  erroneous  impression.  Un- 
doubtedly, the  majority  of  adolescents  in  a  particular  culture 
eventually  become  full-fledged  adults,  abiding  by  the  major  norms 
of  their  culture,  class,  and  so  on,  just  as  the  "pleasant  co-operative" 
Manus  children  become  "grasping  competitive  Manus  adults." 
An  adolescent  may  defy  his  parents  in  relation  to  some  more  or 
less  important  norms  of  his  class;  yet  later,  when  he  has  passed 
into  adulthood  and  has  incorporated  norms  appropriate  to  the 
interests  of  his  class  and  the  status  of  adults  in  his  particular  situa- 
tion, he  may  staunchly  uphold  the  very  same  norms  he  once  so 
violently  repudiated.  For  example,  George  Apley  of  John  Mar- 
quand's  novel  [17]  outraged  his  Boston  Brahmin  family  by  falling 
in  love  with  an  Irish  girl.  Only  after  the  onslaught  of  united 
family  pressure  and  a  trip  to  Europe  did  he  give  up  his  intentions 


220  RE-FORMATION  OF  THE  EGO  IN  ADOLESCENCE 

of  marrying  her.  But  years  later,  George,  now  in  his  father's  shoes, 
fought  with  almost  equal  intensity  his  own  son's  affair  with  a 
daughter  of  a  nouveau  riche  from  Worcester. 

These  few  examples  of  cultural  variation  could  be  multiplied  by 
hundreds  from  the  rather  disconnected  studies  of  ethnologists. 
The  facts  reported  here  should  be  enough  to  give  us  some  perspec- 
tive and  save  us  from  the  error  of  making  generalizations  based 
on  particular  cultures.  All  in  all,  a  survey  of  this  material  indi- 
cates that  the  problems  facing  the  adolescent  vary  from  culture  to 
culture,  rendering  the  transition  to  adulthood  more  or  less  compli- 
cated, more  or  less  conflicting,  more  or  less  prolonged.  Such 
studies  indicate  the  necessity  of  using  comparative  material  from 
different  cultures  and  times,  and  the  necessity  of  first  placing 
adolescent  ego  problems  in  their  social  settings.  For  significant 
variations  and  factors  of  social  change  necessarily  reflect  them- 
selves in  the  status  problems  of  the  adolescent,  who  are  themselves 
in  a  critical  and  unstable  stage  of  transition.  After  learning  this 
from  the  ethnologist,  we  can  return  to  our  own  work  as  psycholo- 
gists carrying  with  us  the  implications  of  the  lesson  learned.  We 
should  start  our  work  by  reiterating  a  methodological  considera- 
tion appropriate  in  this  connection.  With  variations  in  the  social 
setting,  the  transitional  period  of  adolescence  may  be  more  or  less 
prolonged,  fraught  with  more  or  less  intense  problems.  However, 
the  basic  psychological  principles  which  operate  in  all  of  these 
social  settings  should  be  the  same.  If  we  abandon  the  search  for 
these  psychological  principles,  we  will  be  falling  into  the  tragic 
blunder  committed  by  the  Fascist  advocates  of  basic  race  differences. 

REFERENCES 

1.  BOLL,  E.  S.,  Britain's  experience  with  adolescents,  'Ann.  'Am.  TAcad.  Pol.  and  Soc. 

Sci.,  1944,  236,  74-82. 

2.  BOSSARD,  J.  H.  S.,  Family  backgrounds  of  wartime  adolescents,  Ann.  Am.  Acad. 

Pol.  and  Soc.  Sci.,  1944,  236,  33-42. 

3.  BROWN,  A.  R.,  Andaman  Islanders,  Cambridge  (England) :  Univ.  Press,  1922. 

4.  DAVIS,  K.,  Youth  in  the  Depression,  Chicago:  Univ.  Press,  copyright  1935. 

5. Adolescence  and  the  social  structure,  Ann.  Am.  Acad.  Pol.  and  Soc.  Sci., 

1944,  236,  8-16. 
6.  GARDNER,  G.  E.,  Sex  behavior  of  adolescents  in  wartime,  rAnn.  Am.  'Acad.  Pol. 

and  Soc.  Set.,  1944,  236,  60-6. 


REFERENCES  221 

7.  GROVES,  E.  R.,  and  G.  H.  GROVES,  The  social  background  of  wartime  adoles- 

cents, Ann.  Am.  Acad.  Pol.  and  Soc.  Sci.,  1944,  236,  26-32. 

8.  HALL.  G.  SM  Adolescence,  Its  Psychology  and  Us  Relations  to  Physiology,  An- 

thropology, Sociology,  Sex,  Crime,  Religion  and  Education,  New  York: 
Appleton,  copyright  1904. 

9.  HANKINS,  D.,  Mental  hygiene  problems  of  the  adolescent  period,  Ann.  Am. 

Acad.  Pol.  and  Soc.  Sci.,  1944,  236,  128-35. 

10.  JONES,  H.  E,,  Development  in  Adolescence,  New  York:  Appleton-Century, 

copyright  1943. 

11.  KLINEBERG,  O.,  Race  Differences,  New  York:  Harper,  copyright  1935. 
12. Social  Psychology,  New  York:  Holt,  copyright  1940. 

13.  KOMAROVSKY,  M.,  The  Unemployed  Man  and  His  Family,  New  York:  Dryden, 

1940. 

14.  LYND,  R.  S.,  and  H.  M,  LYND,  Middletown  in  Transition,  New  York:  Harcourt, 

Brace,  copyright  1937. 

15.  MAGEE,  E.  S.,  Impact  of  the  war  on  child  labor,  Ann.  'Am.  Acad.  Pol.  and  Soc. 

Sci.,  1944,  236,  101-09. 

16.  MAJUMDAR,  D.  N.,  A  Tribe  in  Transition,  New  York:  Longmans,  Green,  1937. 

17.  MARQUAND,  J.  P.,  The  Late  George  Apley,  Boston:  Little,  Brown,  1937. 

18.  MCGRANAHAN,  D.  V.,  and  M.  JANOWITZ,  Studies  of  German  youth,  /.  Abnorm. 

&  Soc.  Psychol,  1946, 41,  3-14. 

19.  MEAD,  M.,  Prom  the  South  Seas,  Studies  of  Adolescence  and  Sex  in  Primitive 

Societies,  New  York:  Morrow,  copyright  1939. 

20.  : Coming  of  Age  in  Samoa,  in  [19]. 

21. Growing  Up  in  New  Guinea,  in  [19]. 

22. ?  Sex  and  Temperament  in  Three  Primitive  Societies,  in  [19]. 

23.  RADIN,  P.,  The  Autobiography  of  a  Winnebago  Indian,  Univ.  Calif.  Publica- 
tions in  Am.  Archaeology  and  Ethnology,  vol.  16,  April  15,  1920,  381-473. 

24. Primitive  Religion,  Its  Nature  and  Origin,  New  York:  Viking,  copy- 
right 1937. 

25.  RIVERS,  W.  H.  R.,  History  of  Melanesian  Society,  Cambridge  (England) :  Univ. 

Press,  1924. 

26.  SKINNER,  M.,  and  A.  S.  NUTT,  Adolescents  away  from  home,  'Ann.  Am.  Acad. 

Pol.  and  Soc.  Sci.,  1944,  236,  51-9. 

27.  SORENSON,  R.,  Wartime  recreation  for  adolescents,  Ann.  Am.  fAcad.  Pol.  and 

Soc.  Sci.,  1944,  236,  145-51. 

28.  VAN  GENNEP,  A.,  Les  Rites  de  passage,  Paris,  1909. 

29.  WEAVER,  P.,  Youth  and  religion,  Ann.  Am.  Acad.  Pol.  and  Soc.  Sci.,  1944, 

236, 152-60. 

30.  WEBSTER,  H.,  Taboo,  A  Sociological  Study,  Stanford:  Univ.  Press,  1942. 

31.  WHITING,  J.  W.  M.,  Becoming  a  Kwoma,  New  Haven:  Yale  Univ.  Press, 

copyright  194L 

32.  WILLIAMS,  F.,  Russia,  Youth  and  the  Present  Day  World,  New  York:  Farrar  & 

Rinehart,  1934. 

33.  ZACHRY,  C.  B.,  Customary  stresses  and  strains  of  adolescence,  Ann.  'Am.  rAcad. 

Pol.  and  Soc.  Sci.,  1944,  236,  136-44. 


CHAPTER  9 

RE-FORMATION  OF  THE  EGO  IN  ADOLESCENCE 

(Continued) 

We  have  seen  that  by  the  time  of  puberty  the  child  develops  to 
relate  himself  psychologically  to  definite  groups,  such  as  family, 
school,  age  mates,  sex  groups,  church,  and  other  social  institutions. 
We  have  cited  briefly  some  effects  of  these  diverse  identifications. 
But  the  child  has  yet  to  achieve  the  status  of  a  full-fledged  adult 
man  or  woman  with  all  that  that  status  implies  in  the  way  of 
grown-up  functions,  rights,  privileges,  and  responsibilities.  Psy- 
chologically, he  has  yet  to  be  treated  as  adult  by  his  social  sur- 
roundings. He  has  yet  to  experience  himself  and  behave  as  adult 
through  the  incorporation  of  the  grown-up  values  of  masculinity 
or  femininity,  norms  of  some  work  or  profession  (or  anticipations 
thereof),  and  norms  of  social  status  as  prescribed  by  the  prevailing 
values  of  the  economic  and  social  institutions  within  his  reach. 
Usually,  the  period  of  adolescence  during  which  girls  and  boys 
reach  their  major  physiological  maturity  is  the  period  of  transition 
when  they  face  these  problems.  The  relative  ease  or  difficulty,  the 
duration  of  the  transition  depends,  as  was  briefly  indicated  in  the 
last  chapter,  on  the  particular  economic  and  social  circumstances 
surrounding  the  boys  or  girls.  The  variations  of  socioeconomic 
conditions,  the  integrated  or  contradictory  character  of  prevailing 
norms,  the  consistency  or  inconsistency  of  the  treatment  and  ex- 
pectations faced,  the  integration  or  "marginality"  of  their  own 
roles  as  part  of  the  general  social  scheme,  enter  in  as  important 
factors. 

THE  DEVELOPING  EGO  RENDERED  UNSTABLE  AND,  AT  TIMES,  CRITICAL 

Young  boys  and  girls  strive  to  satisfy  newly  developing  desires, 
to  get  settled  or  start  getting  settled.  In  the  many  different  ways 

222 


DEVELOPING  EGO  RENDERED  UNSTABLE  AND,  AT  TIMES,  CRITICAL     223 

in  which  they  experience  themselves  as  being  involved,  they  make 
effective  or  futile  efforts  of  emancipation  or  independence.  In 
their  strivings  to  do  away  with  childish  things,  their  "psychological 
weaning" 1  or  drifting  away  from  earlier  dependencies  is  manifested 
in  various  ways.  In  the  course  of  these  strivings,  there  arise  in- 
stabilities, insecurities,  adult-youth  conflicts,  and  crises  proportional 
to  the  resistances  and  inconsistencies  met  in  the  surroundings. 
These  resulting  instabilities  and  insecurities  are,  of  course,  subject 
to  variations  due  to  individual  differences. 

Our  present  concern  is  die  impact  of  these  phenomena  on  the 
developing  ego-attitudes.  For  the  developing  ego,  which  is  thus 
rendered  less  stable,  less  secure,  wavering,  and  conflicting  in  the 
midst  of  transition  and  inconsistent  situations  and  norms,  has  to  be 
related  anew  to  a  constellation  of  more  mature  personal  and  group 
relationships.  It  is  painful  to  toss  around  without  some  stable 
anchorage.  Even  the  satisfaction  of  basic  drives,  including  die 
newly  developed  definite  sex  urges,  have  to  be  justified  in  terms  of 
one's  ego-values.  Because  of  the  lack  of  adequate  situations  pro- 
vided by  the  established  grown-up  world  which  might  make  these 
status  anchorings  possible,  the  result,  in  many  cases,  is  the  sponta- 
neous formation  of  more  or  less  secret  cliques,  gangs,  and  similar 
adolescent  groups.  We  shall  designate  such  groups  as  membership 
groups.  In  other  cases,  crushes  or  identifications  with  idolized 
persons,  or  identifications  with  groups  of  which  the  adolescent  is 
not  directly  a  member  (or  even  with  whom  he  is  not  in  contact) 
may  serve  a  similar  function  psychologically.  These  latter  identi- 
fications will  be  designated  as  reference  idols  or  reference  groups.2 
These  membership  groups,  reference  idols,  and  reference  groups 
play  an  important  role  in  determining  the  interests,  attitudes,  and 
ego  links  of  the  adolescent  boy  or  girl.  In  the  remainder  of  this 
chapter,  we  shall  give  an  account  of  the  ego  development  of  ado- 
lescents along  these  lines.  Our  account  will  necessarily  be  sketchy. 

1  This  characterization  was  aptly  used  and  elaborated  by  L.  Hollingworth.  [20, 
36  ff.;  21,  882-408] 

2  The  term  reference  group  was  used  by  Hyman  (pp.  137/.).    The  exact  sense 
in  which  we  use  it  will  become  clear  as  we  go  along.    Our  use  of  it  is  not  essen- 
tially different  from  Hyman's. 


224  RE-FORMATION  OF  THE  EGO  IN  ADOLESCENCE 

The  characterization  of  adolescence  as  the  period  of  transition  to 
adult  status,  and,  consequently,  to  mature  or  grown-up  ego-atti- 
tudes certainly  needs  to  be  qualified.  For  under  prevailing  eco- 
nomic conditions  a  boy  may  be  drawn  to  work  in  a  shop,  plant,  or 
farm  even  some  years  before  the  advent  of  puberty;  a  girl  may  be 
imposed  with  the  serious  responsibilities  of  keeping  house  or 
taking  care  of  children.  In  such  situations,  he  or  she  does  develop 
adult  ego-attitudes  in  these  respects.  Still,  if  the  age  mates  of  a 
child  in  such  situations  are  not  undergoing  similar  experiences,  he 
or  she  may  experience  more  or  less  serious  conflicts.  And,  while 
performing  the  tasks  of  an  adult,  he  or  she  may  in  other  ways  be 
treated  as  a  mere  child.  Actual  observations  in  several  Turkish 
villages  have  provided  many  concrete  illustrations  of  such  treat- 
ment. Before  they  were  even  seven  or  eight,  these  Turkish  chil- 
dren showed  hardly  any  remnants  of  childish  egocentric  speech. 
In  some  respects,  they  behaved  and  spoke  as  prematurely  grown-up 
children.  The  coefficient  of  egocentricity  seemed  to  be  reduced 
almost  to  zero  under  the  mature  responsibilities  they  were  sub- 
jected to.  Nevertheless,  this  premature  development  in  some  re- 
spects did  not  save  them  from  adolescent  problems  after  they 
reached  puberty.  We  observed  cases  of  various  kinds  of  parent- 
youth  conflict,  problems  related  to  marriage  and  sexual  activities 
due  to  adolescent  physiological  development.  For  example,  in 
some  cases,  forced  or  mutually  agreed-upon  elopements,  with  a 
great  many  complications  from  the  family  and  other  sources,  en- 
sued as  a  consequence.  These  are  only  illustrations.  Many  other 
variations  of  adolescent  problems  due  to  differential  inconsistent 
out-of-phase  timings  of  the  various  developmental  processes  may 
be  found. 

Changing  body  and  changing  self.*  In  observations  concerned 
with  the  very  beginning  of  ego  development,  we  saw  the  impor- 
tance and  primacy  of  the  delineation  of  the  baby's  body  as  his 
from  surrounding  objects,  the  recognition  of  his  hands,  feet,  face, 
as  his.  Up  to  the  advent  of  puberty,  boys  and  girls  are  certainly 
preoccupied  with  their  bodies.  As  may  be  seen  in  examples  se- 
lected at  random  from  hundreds  of  cases,  boys  and  girls,  during 

8  This  phrase,  used  by  Zachry  [49]  as  a  chapter  heading,  apdy  epitomizes  the 
adolescent  bodily  changes  and  their  psychological  consequences.  [31] 


DEVELOPING  EGO  RENDERED  UNSTABLE  AND,  AT  TIMES,  CRITICAL     225 

adolescence,  become  acutely  aware  of  their  own  bodies  and  even 
parts  of  it  (besides  the  sex  organs),  especially  in  a  society  where 
there  is  a  high  premium  on  physical  attractiveness.  An  adoles- 
cent's whole  ego  concern  may  at  times  be  focused  on  the  attrac- 
tiveness or,  real  or  fancied,  unattractiveness  of  even  a  part  of  the 
body.  Prevailing  norms  concerning  feminine  and  masculine  at- 
tractiveness and  the  relative  importance  attached  to  these  norms  in 
the  person's  reference  and  membership  groups  may,  of  course, 
determine  in  significant  ways  the  extent  and  intensity  of  ego- 
involvements  focused  on  the  body  or  its  parts. 

Studies  dealing  with  physiological  developments  and  their  psy- 
chological correlates  at  the  period  of  adolescence  are  being  rapidly 
accumulated.  After  mentioning  briefly  the  physiological  changes, 
we  shall  concern  ourselves  mainly  with  their  impact  on  ego  de- 
velopment. A  few  more  or  less  representative  accounts  of  these 
changes  may  be  found  in  the  references  noted. 

The  bodily  changes  occurring  during  adolescence  are  regulated 
to  an  important  degree  by  the  endocrine  glands.  The  pituitary, 
thyroid,  adrenal  (cortex),  and  gonads  play  particularly  important 
roles  in  the  growth  phenomena  of  this  period.  Adequate  function- 
ing of  all  the  endocrine  glands,  with  their  proper  interrelatedness 
with  each  other,  with  the  nervous  and  the  cardiovascular  systems, 
is  apparently  a  necessary  condition  for  normal  growth.  Under 
the  regulation  of  the  endocrine  glands,  an  increased  growth  in 
height  and  weight  generally  precedes  pubescence.  This  growth  is 
accompanied  by  changes  in  body  proportions  toward  more  typi- 
cally masculine  and  feminine  builds,  changes  in  the  size  of  most 
of  the  internal  organs,  appearance  of  the  secondary  sex  charac- 
teristics (for  example,  growth  of  pubic  hair;  change  of  voice  and 
growth  of  beard  in  boys;  breast  development  in  girls).  At  the 
same  time  there  is  a  maturation  of  the  reproductive  organs  of  each 
sex,  ordinarily  indicated  by  the  onset  of  menstruation  for  girls  and 
the  secretion  of  spermatozoa  for  boys,  together  with  the  growth 
and  development  of  the  genitalia.  Such  changes  do  not  occur,  of 
course,  without  some  changes  in  the  general  functioning  of  the 
bodily  processes,  as  indicated,  for  example,  by  measurements  of 
basal  metabolic  rate. 


226  RE-FORMATION  OF  THE  EGO  IN  ADOLESCENCE 

The  psychological  consequences  of  these  gross  changes  and  the 
more  or  less  clear-cut  differentiation  of  the  sexes  at  this  time  are 
treated  later  in  more  detail.  However,  we  should  note  here  for 
future  reference  two  of  the  more  important  facts  of  physical 
growth  for  our  problem  of  the  re-formation  of  the  ego  at  adoles- 
cence. First,  girls,  on  the  average,  mature  physiologically  a  year 
or  so  earlier  than  boys.  As  we  shall  see,  the  psychological  conse- 
quence of  this  fact  is  that  girls  tend  to  form  attitudes  of  adult 
femininity  and  social  maturity  earlier  than  boys  of  the  same  age. 
We  sometimes  find,  for  example,  that  girls  tend  to  drag  the  boys 
of  their  age  with  them  into  more  mature  interests  and  skills,  such 
as  dancing,  appropriate  to  their  more  mature  level  of  development. 

Secondly,  not  only  does  variation  in  time  and  rate  of  growth  and 
the  onset  of  pubescence  vary  from  environment  to  environment, 
from  class  to  class,  but  even  within  a  given  group,  individual 
variations  in  development  and  rate  of  growth  may  be  marked. 
And  we  shall  see  that  marked  deviation  in  either  direction  from 
group  norms  of  development  has  psychological  consequences  for 
the  adolescent  boy  or  girl  who  is  in  such  a  situation.4 

With  these  more  striking  physiological  changes,  one's  conception 
of  one's  ego  correspondingly  changes.  As  Zachry  [49]  aptly  puts 
it,  "the  body  is  symbolic  of  the  self,"  [32]  To  start  with,  the  very 
psychological  correlates  of  these  changes  tend  to  be  felt  as  extraor- 
dinary even  independent  of  their  ego  relationship.  [47]  The  ado- 
lescent's already  accentuated  awareness  and  focusing  on  his  body 
becomes  even  more  acute  with  the  more  pronounced,  somewhat 
stylized  attention  of  others  (for  example,  parents  and  other  adoles- 
cents) on  his  or  her  body,  with  sex  desires  toward  and  from  age 
mates  now  present  in  a  developed  way.  And  with  the  develop- 
ment of  these  sex  desires  and  changing  attitudes  toward  one's  body 
there  comes  an  intense  awareness  of  the  developing  opposite  sex. 
This  fact  is  aptly  illustrated  by  the  statements  of  one  boy  concern- 
ing his  changing  associations  with  girls.  In  junior  high  school 
this  boy  was  only  "interested"  in  girls.  However,  he  writes: 

4  These  bodily  changes  during  adolescence  are  described  in  detail  in  [2,  3,  4,  7, 
18,  20,  22,  25,  37,  39,  47,  49];  the  earlier  development  of  girls  is  discussed  in 
[2,  4,  7,  39,  49] ;  variations  of  growth  are  shown  in  [1,  2,  4,  20,  49]. 


DEVELOPING  EGO  RENDERED  UNSTABLE  AND,  AT  TIMES,  CRITICAL     227 

It  was  in  high  school  that  I  began  to  date  in  the  true  sense  of  the 
word.  It  was  now  that  girls  were  beginning  to  attract  me  in  a  differ- 
ent light.  It  was  no  longer  their  intelligence  or  athletic  ability  or 
torn-boyishness  which  attracted  me,  but  it  was  the  way  they  wore  their 
makeup,  the  way  they  moved  their  hips  when  they  walked,  and  the 
way  they  snuggled  up  close  when  we  danced  that  I  began  to  notice. 
It  was  all  new,  strange,  scary,  but  terribly  interesting  and  exciting. 
[48, 12] 

As  a  consequence  of  these  changes,  the  adolescent  cannot  help 
but  preoccupy  himself  and,  especially,  herself  with  comparisons  in 
relation  to  the  prevailing  norms  of  body  proportions  and  growth, 
and,  more  specifically,  in  relation  to  the  girls  or  boys  of  his  own 
group.  Zachry  mentions  the  case  of  a  girl  who,  being  slightly 
"larger  and  heavier  than  most  of  her  contemporaries/'  became  so 
conscious  of  her  body  that  she  covered  herself  up  most  of  the 
time  "in  a  voluminous  smock."  [49,  63]  Frequent  letters  appear 
in  the  interesting  magazine  Seventeen  to  this  effect: 

How  about  showing  some  clothes  for  the  short,  chubby  teen-ager? 
We  all  want  to  look  like  the  rest  of  the  crowd — but  you  know  how 
some  of  us  look  in  sweaters  and  skirts. 

Or 

You  put  too  much  emphasis  on  the  petite  junior  miss  and  leave 
her  skyscraper  cousin  out  in  the  coldl 

The  adolescent  boy  or  girl  may  try  very  hard  to  incorporate  in 
himself  or  herself  and  to  conform  to  the  particular  norms  of  femi- 
ninity or  masculinity  fashionable  in  the  surroundings  when  sexual 
maturity  has  been  reached.  At  first  these  efforts  may  be  clumsy 
or  affected.  And  all  of  these  strivings  tend  to  produce  feelings  of 
uncertainty  about  one's  own  self. 

In  nearly  all  the  representative  works  on  adolescence,  there  are 
many  concrete  illustrations  of  the  changing  body  and  the  changing 
self,  and  a  more  or  less  acute  focusing  of  the  whole  conception  of 
one's  self  on  the  body  in  general,  or  even  on  some  part  of  the  body. 
This  may  become  psychologically  so  exaggerated  as  to  become  a 
major  ego  concern  and  may  have  a  major  influence  in  regulating 


228  RE-FORMATION  OF  THE  EGO  IN  ADOLESCENCE 

the  adolescent's  social  behavior.  Even  anxieties  and  insecurities  in 
other  respects  may  be  reflected  in  such  a  concern.  Peter  Bios  [5] 
gives  the  following  illustration: 

At  the  age  of  thirteen  or  fourteen  years,  Betty  begins  to  be  very 
concerned  with  her  looks.  The  mole  on  her  chee\,  which  is  some- 
what noticeable,  becomes  the  focus  of  her  self-consciousness.  Worry 
about  her  appearance  increases  and  finally  motivates  her  to  avoid 
dances  or  parties  until  the  mole  will  have  been  removed  by  an  opera- 
tion. She  has  read  about  operations  which  have  succeeded  in  im- 
proving the  looks  of  women,  and  has  set  her  hopes  on  this  surgical 
work.  [5,  93,  italics  ours] 

To  cite  just  a  few  other  cases  of  the  body  or  its  parts  becoming 
a  major  focus  of  the  adolescent's  ego,  the  following  statements, 
communicated  to  the  writers  recently  by  a  former  student  of  a 
large  Midwestern  University,  are  relevant: 

All  through  my  grade  school  years,  I  was  the  tallest  girl  in  the  class, 
and,  as  a  matter  of  fact,  was  rather  proud  of  it.  However,  the  summer 
before  I  entered  Junior  High  School  at  the  age  of  thirteen,  I  grew  to 
just  an  inch  below  my  present  five-feet-eight.  I  towered  above  every 
girl  and  boy  in  our  class;  and  it  seemed  to  me  that  I  was  the  tallest 
girl  in  the  world.  None  of  my  family's  comforting  words  made  it 
easier  for  me  to  wall(  across  the  room  at  school.  In  high  school,  two 
girls  taller  than  I  entered  the  class.  But  they  weren't  in  our  crowd, 
so  I  continued  to  feel  like  a  giraffe  when  I  went  out  with  the  girls. 
I  suppose  the  fact  that  several  of  the  boys  grew  to  six-footers  helped 
dispel  that  shrinking  feeling.  But  the  crowning  touch  came  in  the 
spring  of  my  junior  year.  The  school  paper  published  a  list  of 
characteristics  of  a  composite  "Ideal  Girl."  Lo  and  behold,  my  name 
was  listed  after  "Ideal  Height."  I  haven't  felt  too  tall  since,  [italics 
ours] 

Averill  [1]  mentions  another  case  of  a  comparatively  tall  girl  who 
walked  stooped  and  with  round  shoulders  in  order  to  appear 
shorter.  [32]  Hollingworth  [20]  mentions  several  examples,  in- 
cluding the  absorption  of  some  adolescents  with  pimpled  or  blem- 
ished skin.  [8]  One  boy  began  to  walk  on  tiptoes  because  his  feet 
seemed  so  large  [12];  another  was  so  conscious  of  his  changing 
voice  that  he  refused,  even  under  pressure  from  the  teacher,  to 
sing  in  school  [13] ;  and  still  another  sat  "with  his  hand  over  his 


DEVELOPING  EGO  RENDERED  UNSTABLE  AND,  AT  TIMES,  CRITICAL     229 

mouth  and  chin  much  of  the  time"  because  he  thought  he  needed 
to  shave  and  his  parents  disagreed.  [13] 

As  can  easily  be  seen  in  the  foregoing  illustrations,  the  problems 
raised  in  regard  to  one's  self  are  relative  matters;  that  is,  problems 
arise  in  regard  to  one's  own  stage  of  maturity  and  development 
(height,  weight,  bodily  proportions,  and  the  like),  in  relation  to 
one's  own  sex  and  age  mates.  In  several  studies  carried  out  in  the 
Institute  of  Child  Welfare  of  the  University  of  California,  which 
has  contributed  some  of  the  most  significant  investigations  in  the 
field,  this  result  is  reported  time  and  again.  For  example,  Nancy 
Bayley  and  R.  Tuddenham  [4]  conclude: 

.  .  .  that  being  different  from  their  peers  seemed  to  be  a  potential 
hazard  to  adequate  adjustment.  The  poorest  adjusted  among  the  jour 
extreme  groups  were  the  early-maturing  girls  and  the  late-maturing 
boys.  As  these  children  attended  a  coeducational  school  in  which 
grade  placement  was  largely  by  chronological  age,  the  two  groups  who 
would  stand  out  in  the  schoolroom  as  physically  most  different  would 
be  the  large,  early-maturing  girls  and  the  small,  late-maturing  boys. 
To  be  a  girl  and  large,  and  to  be  a  boy  and  small,  are  both  contrary 
to  the  ideals  of  the  culture,  and  therefore  it  seems  plausible  to  expect 
that  some  of  these  children,  if  they  do  not  have  adequate  compensa- 
tions in  other  fields,  might  well  find  their  physical  difference  an 
emotional  problem.  [4,  53,  italics  ours] 

Similarly,  Zachry  states: 

A  boy  whose  physical  development  begins  rather  earlier  than  that 
of  other  boys  in  his  class  is  apt  to  feel  not  only  that  his  body  is  out- 
sized.  Perhaps  he  comes  to  feel  himself  a  misfit.  [49,  51,  italics  ours] 

A  case  cited  by  Averill  illustrates  this  point.  Harold  became 
pubescent  before  the  boys  in  his  "gang"  of  which  he  had  been  the 
leader. 

.  .  .  Harold's  lengthening  arms  and  legs,  and  his  amazingly  enlarg- 
ing hands  and  feet  have  made  him  decidedly  awkward  and  clumsy. 
The  other  boys,  not  yet  pubescent,  are  beginning  to  be  somewhat 
shaken  in  their  loyalties  to  their  recent  leader.  [1,  31] 

Peter  Bios  illustrates  the  same  general  fact  conversely. 

The  late  developing  boy  or  the  boy  showing  inappropriate  sex  de- 
velopment is  handicapped  in  his  social  development  on  account  of 


230  RE-FORMATION  OF  THE  EGO  IN  ADOLESCENCE 

group  discrimination.  It  has  been  observed  that  changes  in  physical 
status  are  followed  by  a  changed  attitude  of  the  group:  thus  a  boy 
with  retarded  maturation  was  long  an  outsider  until  a  spurt  of  growth 
set  in  which  subsequently  led  to  his  smooth  absorption  in  the  group. 
[5,  253,  italics  ours] 

The  self-consciousness  due  to  late  maturation  in  relation  to  one's 
own  age  mates  may  cause  the  adolescent  greater  or  lesser  degrees 
of  shyness  or  timidity  in  his  behavior.  F.  W.  Burks  [9]  reports  in 
a  study  of  the  Tugwell  High  School  Clubhouse  (1936-37)  5  in 
California  that  the  "late  maturing  boys  were  not  able  to  utilize  the 
clubhouse  as  a  laboratory  for  practicing  techniques  for  use  with 
girls  in  quite  so  free  an  atmosphere  as  those  who  had  achieved 
some  proficiency  in  the  Jackson  School  clubhouse."  [12]  In 
White's  account  of  Joseph  Kidd  [46]  we  see  a  concrete  example 
of  the  difficulties  encountered  by  a  boy  who  developed  later  than 
his  classmates,  in  this  case  because  he  was  nearly  two  years  their 
junior.  White  states  Joseph's  predicament: 

Being  in  the  same  grade  with  his  brother,  he  had  come  to  depend 
upon  him  for  companionship,  initiative,  and  even  defense.  When 
sexual  maturity  carried  the  brother  into  a  new  circle  of  activities,  Kidd 
felt  deserted  and  helpless;  he  was  faced  by  the  new  task  of  making 
his  own  way,  a  task  for  which  his  being  more  than  a  year  younger 
than  the  group  was  at  this  age  a  serious  handicap.  His  accustomed 
social  attitudes  were  now  revealed  as  wholly  unsuitable.  [46,  796] 

The  relative  standing  of  the  development  of  an  adolescent  in 
relation  to  his  age  mates  may  not  be  a  constant  matter;  it  may 
mean  different  things  in  different  situations.  Zachry  reports  an 
illustration  of  this  point  "as  described  by  Lawrence  S.  Kubie,  M.D., 
at  a  meeting  held  by  the  Study  of  Adolescents." 

At  the  age  of  twelve  one  youngster  may,  for  example,  attain  a  size 
more  usual  to  a  boy  of  sixteen.  Grouped  with  other  twelve-year-olds 
in  the  classroom  he  may  feel  quite  comfortable,  since  he  is  not  unlike 
them  in  the  stage  of  his  intellectual  and  emotional  development.  But 
among  the  same  boys  on  the  playground  he  is  facing  a  curiously 
complicated  psychological  challenge.  [49,  50] 

6  We  are  indebted  to  Drs.  H.  E.  Jones  and  M.  C.  Jones  for  securing  this  un- 
published report. 


DEVELOPING  EGO  RENDERED  UNSTABLE  AND,  AT  TIMES,  CRITICAL     231 

In  detailed  investigations  of  various  aspects  of  the  adolescence  of 
John  Sanders,  one  of  the  cases  studied  longitudinally  by  the  Insti- 
tute of  Child  Welfare  of  the  University  of  California  [25],  we  find 
clear-cut  substantiation  of  the  importance  of  the  changing  body 
and  changing  self  in  relation  to  one's  own  group.  John,  who.  was 
not  an  unusual  boy,  whose  case  was  not  a  dramatic  one,  and  who, 
incidentally,  could  get  along  much  better  with  his  father  than  with 
his  mother,  was  rated  by  his  classmates  as  approximately  average 
in  different  personal  characteristics  (masculine,  happy,  popular, 
and  so  on)  at  the  age  of  12.5  years.  John  matured  physically  a 
year  or  so  later  than  his  age  mates. 

.  .  .  during  his  junior-high-school  years  John  became  markedly 
shorter,  lighter,  and  punier  in  relation  to  classmates.  [25,  155  f.] 

At  the  age  of  15,  John  suffered  a  "disheartening  change  of  status 
in  all  the  traits  represented."  [154] 

Adult  observers  agreed  that  around  the  age  of  15  John  was  at  a  low 
point  in,  for  example,  popularity,  initiative,  and  good-naturedness, 
and  at  a  high  point  in  evidences  of  anxiety,  show-off  behavior,  and 
affectation.  [25,  755] 

His  "adolescent  spurt"  started  at  15,  a  year  or  so  later  than  other 
boys.  With  this  spurt,  an  upward  trend  came  in  his  sociability  and 
status  in  relation  to  his  age  mates.  By  the  age  of  17.6  he  ap- 
proached the  average  of  his  age  mates.  In  his  freshman  year  in 
college,  John  was  reported  as  having  "a  pleasing  personality." 
In  analyzing  John's  case,  Harold  Jones  states: 

.  .  .  delayed  maturing  may  lead  not  only  to  loss  of  status  with 
others,  but  also  to  the  anxiety  expressed  in  the  question,  "Am  I  nor- 
mal?" When  the  biological  innovations  of  adolescence  are  at  last 
clearly  avowed,  a  turning  point  may  be  reached  not  merely  in  physio- 
logical development,  but  also  in  social  recognition  and  in  feelings  of 
personal  security.  The  interpretation  followed  above  stresses  the  social 
significance  of  adolescent  changes,  and  implies  that  the  psychological 
effect  of  these  changes  rests  upon  the  degree  to  which  an  individual 
is  sensitive  to  the  norms  and  values  of  his  social  environment  [25, 
756] 


232  RE-FORMATION  OF  THE  EGO  IN  ADOLESCENCE 

Incorporation  in  the  ego  of  norms  of  masculinity  and  femininity. 
We  have  seen  that  as  the  body  changes  into  one  of  a  mature  male 
or  female,  the  adolescent's  conception  of  himself  or  herself  also 
changes.  The  adolescent  begins  to  conceive  of  himself  or  herself 
as  man  or  woman.  This  includes  the  social  expectations  and 
values  attached  to  the  male  or  female  body.  As  some  of  the  illus- 
trations in  the  previous  section  have  indicated,  certain  values  have 
to  do  with  desirable  body  proportions  of  one's  own  and  of  the 
opposite  sex.  These  values,  when  incorporated  as  part  of  the  ego, 
regulate  in  an  important  way  the  adolescent's  experience  and  be- 
havior related  to  his  own  body  and  to  his  choice  of  "desirable" 
companions  of  the  opposite  sex.  As  we  shall  see  later,  these  choices 
are  also  defined  in  terms  of  ego-attitudes  related  to  norms  of  the 
"right"  people  for  associates,  in  terms  of  class,  status,  and  the  like. 
Whereas  girls,  prior  to  adolescence,  are  prone  to  be  fairly  free  in 
the  way  they  handle  their  bodies,  they  now  become  highly  con- 
scious of  them,  handling  them  in  terms  of  such  culturally  pre- 
scribed norms  as  modesty  and  feminine  attractiveness.  So,  too, 
boys  handle  and  treat  their  bodies  in  terms  of  norms  of  masculine 
attractiveness  which  have  now  become  more  acutely  ego-involved 
for  them.  Thus,  a  high  school  boy  may  appear  casual,  athletic,  or 
whatever  stance  and  pose  is  suitable  to  the  particular  culture  and 
time.  Fashions  of  male  and  female  dress  assume  great  importance 
at  this  time.  This  development  has  been  aptly  summarized  for 
200  adolescent  boys  and  girls  by  investigators  at  the  University  of 
California,  [41] 

Psychologically  also  the  girl  feels  a  necessity  of  proving  to  herself 
and  to  the  world  that  she  is  essentially  feminine;  the  boy  needs  to 
demonstrate  that  he  has  those  masculine  qualities  which  require  others 
to  recognize  him  as  a  man.  This  characteristic  accounts  for  the  girls 
spending  a  large  part  of  their  leisure  time  in  shopping  and  in  personal 
adornment.  This  is  the  secret  of  the  manicured  nails,  painted  red  to 
match  vivid  lips.  This  is  why  they  must  wave  and  curl  their  hair, 
and,  having  perfected  the  process,  must  pin  into  it  ribbon  bows,  bits 
of  lace,  or  flowers.  This  is  the  reason  for  the  boy's  urge  to  learn  to 
drive  a  car  and  for  his  willingness  to  move  heaven  and  earth  to  bor- 
row or  own  one.  Along  with  this  development,  also,  we  are  told  by 
our  group  that  a  girl  to  be  popular  must  be  modishly  pretty,  keep 


DEVELOPING  EGO  RENDERED  UNSTABLE  AND,  AT  TIMES,  CRITICAL     233 

herself  clean  and  neat,  be  a  good  mixer.  A  boy,  on  the  other  hand, 
must  be  aggressive  and  must  excel  at  sports.  He  must  have  the  ability 
to  dance  and  to  talk  easily  with  girls,  and  in  addition  he  must  show 
that  he  can  compete  readily  with  other  boys;  that  he  can  achieve  and 
master.  [41,  6/.] 

It  may  be  seen  from  this  summary  characterization  that  not  all 
of  the  social  values  incorporated  as  ego-values  connected  with 
masculinity-femininity  are  directly  related  to  the  male  or  female 
body.  There  are  also  norms  regarding  the  social  and  economic 
functions,  the  statuses  of  men  and  women.  And  there  are  norms 
regarding  the  essential  nature  of  men  and  women  which  are  more 
or  less  appropriate  to  those  particular  social  and  economic  func- 
tions and  statuses.  No  matter  what  the  contribution  of  infantile 
sexuality  may  be  in  determining  the  male  or  female  ego,  the  ego- 
values  of  masculinity  or  femininity  are  effectively  acquired  during 
adolescence.  As  Goodenough  [17]  concludes  on  the  basis  of 
standard  studies  in  America: 

Girls,  on  the  average,  earn  their  most  highly  feminine  score  on  the 
M-F  test  when  they  are  in  the  eighth  grade;  boys  make  their  most 
masculine  score  during  the  third  year  of  high  school.  Roughly,  these 
periods  correspond  to  the  usual  age  at  the  attainment  of  puberty. 
Perhaps  the  explanation  for  the  wide  divergence  of  the  sexes  in  psy- 
chological traits  at  that  time  is  a  reflection  of  the  adolescent's  intense 
interest  in  all  matters  that  have  to  do  with  the  establishment  of  his 
status  as  man  or  woman.  [17,  486 /.,  italics  ours] 

In  Tryon's  study  [44]  of  certain  "factors  involved  in  the  task  of 
maintaining  status  with  one's  peers,"  we  find  some  substantiation 
of  Goodenough's  last  point.  Tryon  studied  350  boys  and  girls, 
first  at  the  age  of  12  and  later  at  the  age  of  15,  acquiring  their 
opinions  of  others  in  the  group  by  means  of  a  verbal-picture 
("Guess  Who")  test.  As  Tryon  summarized  her  findings: 

During  the  period  between  ages  twelve  to  fifteen,  values  for  girls 
underwent  some  revolutionary  changes;  values  for  boys  underwent 
relatively  minor  changes,  mainly  in  terms  of  slightly  shifted  emphases. 
[44,565] 

As  an  example,  the  12-year-old  girls  with  high  prestige  in  the 
group  were  described  as  "neat,  attractive  appearance;  friendly  but 


234  RE-FORMATION  OF  THE  EGO  IN  ADOLESCENCE 

rather  demure  and  docile  social  manner;  quiet  good  humor;  and 
controlled  behavior  conforming  to  adult  standards"  [563] ;  whereas 
the  15-year-old  girl  gained  prestige  "either  through  buoyant,  rather 
aggressive  good-fellowship  with  both  boys  and  girls,  or  through 
sophisticated,  glamorous  qualities  which  attract  the  boys."  [565] 

As  we  have  seen,  girls  mature  physiologically  earlier  than  boys. 
In  the  group  Tryon  studied  [43]  "most  of  the  girls  had  passed 
through  the  pubescent  period  at  fifteen  years,  and  probably  less 
than  one-half  of  the  boys  had."  [79]  The  data  illustrate  the 
changing  characteristics  or  "traits"  necessary  for  status  accompany- 
ing the  girls'  changing  bodies,  just  as,  as  we  have  seen,  changing 
interests  come  at  this  time.  It  would  seem  entirely  possible,  as 
Tryon  suggests,  that  if  the  boys  in  this  study  had  been  at  a  com- 
parable level  of  physiological  maturity,  a  similar  shift  in  "desir- 
able" personal  qualities  would  have  appeared  for  them.  Whether 
or  not  the  shift  would  be  as  "revolutionary"  as  that  for  girls  re- 
mains to  be  answered  by  a  study  of  the  question.  It  is  even  pos- 
sible that  a  further  shift  might  be  found  in  the  girls'  values.  For, 
as  Tryon  notes,  the  girls  who  were  rated  high  because  of  "cordial 
rather  dominating  good-fellowship  with  both  boys  and  girls  .  .  . 
were  very  successful  with  the  boys  who  were  just  venturing  into 
mixed-sex  social  situations;  the  behavior  of  these  girls  was  enough 
like  that  of  the  boys  that  it  did  not  alarm  the  boys."  [79] 

The  adolescent  experiences  himself  or  herself  and  behaves  like 
a  man  or  a  woman  as  prescribed  by  these  norms,  whatever  they 
may  be  in  a  particular  culture.  As  Conklin  formulated  it: 

Perceptions  of  the  physical  self  as  male  or  female  and  all  the  possible 
meanings  which  may  accrue  to  those  perceptions  must  cause  a  very 
large  twist  toward  difference  in  the  self  concepts  of  the  two  sexes. 
[12,  56,  italics  ours] 

Certainly,  norms  regarding  the  conceptions  of  man  and  woman, 
their  functions,  their  status  do  "accrue"  and  do  vary  from  culture 
to  culture  as  determined  by  their  socioeconomic  organizations. 
Consequently,  we  find,  in  different  cultures,  different  identifica- 
tions, behavior  manifestations,  different  problems.  As  Conklin 
points  out: 


DEVELOPING  EGO  RENDERED  UNSTABLE  AND,  AT  TIMES,  CRITICAL     235 

If  the  greater  introversion  is  perceived  or  believed  in  by  a  girl,  that 
will  have  its  reflex  upon  her  self  concept  and  all  self-regarding  func- 
tions. If  the  boy  believes  that  he  has  better  control  than  the  girl,  that 
will  affect  his  attitudes  as  much  as  hers.  If  any  or  all  of  the  possible 
emotional  differences  between  the  sexes  are  brought  to  the  attention 
and  perception  of  boys  and  girls,  then  their  self  concepts  will  be 
affected  thereby.  [12,  56] 

This  point,  which  should  be  seriously  accounted  for  in  any  an- 
alysis of  personality,  finds  expression  again  in  Wile's  statement: 
[47] 

Even  the  growing  differentiation  of  male  dominant  aggressiveness 
and  female  dominant  submissiveness  acquires  meaning  and  value  only 
in  terms  of  social  standards  which  set  them  up  as  laudable  patterns 
for  adolescents.  Their  particular  world  defines  the  processes  to  which 
adolescents  shall  submit  and  undertakes  to  guide  them  into  the  essen- 
tial qualities  which  are  deemed  socially  valuable.  [47,  9] 

The  anthropologists,  in  particular  Margaret  Mead,  have  given 
accounts  of  diverse  and  contrasting  norms  related  to  masculinity 
and  femininity  in  different  cultures.  Such  variations  are  not  pe- 
culiarities or  distant  primitive  cultures  alone.  With  variations  of 
socioeconomic  systems,  the  conception  of  man  and  woman  varies. 
For  example,  the  Lynds  [33]  give  the  following  characterization 
of  the  conception  of  man  and  woman  in  America: 

The  worlds  of  the  two  sexes  constitute  something  akin  to  separate 
subcultures.  Each  involves  an  elaborate  assignment  of  roles  to  its 
members  and  the  development  of  preferred  personality  types  empha- 
sizing various  ones  of  the  more  significant  role  attributes.  .  .  .  But 
this  culture  says  not  only  that  men  and  women  do  different  things; 
they  arc  different  kinds  of  people.  Men  are  stronger,  bolder,  less  pure, 
less  refined,  more  logical,  more  reasonable,  more  given  to  seeing  things 
in  the  large,  but  at  home  needing  coddling  and  reassurance,  "like 
little  boys."  Women  are  more  delicate,  stronger  in  sympathy,  under- 
standing, and  insight,  less  mechanically  adept,  more  immersed  in 
petty  detail  and  in  personalities,  and  given  to  "getting  emotional  over 
things."  [33, 176  f.} 

In  the  Soviet  Union,  this  dichotomy  of  personality  characteristics 
of  man  and  woman  is  not  the  case.  That  such  is  the  fact  does  not 


236  RE-FORMATION  OF  THE  EGO  IN  ADOLESCENCE 

have  to  be  demonstrated  by  citations  from  the  studies  of  psycholo- 
gists. Thousands  of  concrete  illustrations  have  appeared  in  printed 
form  about  the  behavior  of  Soviet  women  during  the  fulfillment 
of  the  Five  Year  Plans  and  during  World  War  II,  not  only  in 
the  auxiliary  armed  services  and  factories,  but  also  in  combat 
situations  and  guerrilla  warfare. 

We  have  mentioned  these  sociological  characterizations  of  norms 
concerning  man  and  woman  because  of  their  organic  connection 
with  our  problem.  First  of  all,  adolescents  are  in  a  transitional 
period  in  terms  of  their  status.  If,  as  Zachry  and  others  point 
out,  they  at  the  same  time  receive  inconsistent  treatments  from 
grown-ups,  they  will  have  further  difficulty  in  stabilizing  their 
egos.  Gardner  [16]  writes  of  such  conflict  situations: 

It  might  be  well  to  note  here  in  the  interest  of  tolerance  of  the 
adolescent  that  as  far  as  the  expression  of  maturity  goes,  he  is  "con- 
demned if  he  does  and  condemned  if  he  doesn't"  show  evidence  of 
maturity  of  sexual  interest  and  outlook,  and  this  even  by  his  own 
parents.  That  parents  wish  their  adolescents  to  grow  up  and  be 
independent  and  that  at  the  same  time  they  wish  them  to  continue 
to  be  children  and  dependent  upon  them  has  never  made  the  task  of 
the  adolescent  an  easy  one.  [16,  61] 

Now,  in  addition  to  such  conflict  situations,  adolescents  are  put  in 
even  more  puzzling  circumstances  by  virtue  of  the  fact  that  many 
of  the  norms  relating  to  masculinity  and  femininity,  even  in 
highly  developed  Western  societies,  are  certainly  survivals  of  past 
centuries  and  periods. 

Modern  technological  developments  and  social  changes,  recent 
momentous  events  of  the  last  decades  (such  as  depression  and  war) 
have  brought  about  changes  in  the  social  behavior  of  men  and, 
especially,  of  women.  But  the  norms  concerning  the  "basic  na- 
ture" of  man  and  woman,  their  "real"  function  and  station  in 
society,  still  survive  side  by  side  with  the  necessary  alterations  in 
behavior  forced  by  the  impact  of  these  changes.  The  discrepancy 
between  certain  practices  imposed  by  social  and  technological 
changes  and  these  norms,  which  in  themselves  are  contradictory, 
produces  conflict  situations  which  surely  have  their  psychological 
consequences.  Pointing  to  this  discrepancy,  the  Lynds  state: 


DEVELOPING  EGO  RENDERED  UNSTABLE  AND,  AT  TIMES,  CRITICAL     237 

But  the  modifications  have  been  in  the  kind  of  behavior  sanctioned 
by  the  culture,  not  in  the  belief  that  men  and  women  are  different  in 
character  and  temperament,  and  not  in  the  ways  in  which  they  are 
believed  to  be  different.  The  modifications  of  the  behavior  patterns 
themselves  consist  in  tolerated  exceptions  rather  than  in  the  develop- 
ment of  any  clear  alternatives  meeting  with  group  approval.  For  the 
individual,  the  result  is  frequently  either  that  he  is  caught  in  a  chaos 
of  conflicting  patterns,  none  of  them  wholly  condemned,  but  no  one 
of  them  clearly  approved  and  free  from  confusion;  or,  where  the  group 
sanctions  are  clear  in  demanding  a  certain  role  of  man  or  woman, 
the  individual  encounters  cultural  requirements  with  no  immediate 
means  of  meeting  them.  [33,  777] 

Since  adolescents  are  themselves  in  a  more  or  less  critical  process 
of  transition,  these  situations  certainly  breed  adolescent  insecuri- 
ties, conflicts,  rebellions,  and,  at  times,  inevitable  crises.  In  the 
process  of  re-formation  of  the  ego,  we  should  expect  to  find,  as, 
the  California  study  mentioned  earlier  in  this  section  indicates, 
that 

.  .  .  once  the  girl  has  arrived  at  the  status  in  the  group  to  which 
she  has  aspired,  or  has  learned  to  adjust  herself  to  a  version  of  the 
universal  feminine  model  which  suits  her  own  personality  .  .  .  once 
the  boy  feels  that  he  is  accepted  as  a  man  .  .  .  [he  and  she]  become 
more  stable  and  predictable.  Teachers,  and  parents  say  that  they  have 
"settled  down."  [41,  7] 

However,  the  problems  related  to  the  contradictions  found  in 
being  male  or  female  in  America  do  not  always  disappear  in  early 
adolescence.  We  find  ample  evidence  to  show  that  such  conflicts, 
although  perhaps  lessened  for  a  time,  may  increase  as  the  adoles- 
cent seriously  faces  the  prospects  of  work  and  marriage.  Such 
evidence  is  to  be  found  in  such  studies  as  those  of  Kirkpatrick 
[26,  27]  where  he  reports  that  attitudes  toward  femininity  are 
highly  inconsistent,  both  for  parents  and  adolescents  of  both  sexes. 

It  also  seems  to  be  true  that  the  sex  differences  in  inconsistency  tend 
to  be  greater  in  the  younger  generation.  [26,  355] 

The  fact  that  male  students  "were  decidedly  more  inconsistent 
than  female  students"  would  seem  to  make  the  lot  of  both  males 
and  females  fraught  with  difficulties.  [27,  557] 


238  RE-FORMATION  OF  THE  EGO  IN  ADOLESCENCE 

However,  it  is  the  female  in  a  bourgeois  society  who  is  in  most 
danger  of  remaining  "marginal,"  of  experiencing  continued  con- 
flict due  to  the  contradictory  ego-attitudes  and  situations  in  which 
she  finds  herself.  Kitay  [28]  found,  for  example,  that  girls  tend 
to  incorporate  the  "prevailing  views"  about  themselves  maintained 
by  men,  "even  when  they  are  uncomplimentary."  [405]  In  the 
areas  of  work  opportunities,  these  problems  are  particularly  ac- 
centuated for  girls.  As  Seward  [40]  puts  it: 

Having  provided  its  boys  and  girls  with  the  same  educational  oppor- 
tunities and  vocational  motivation,  our  society  then  reverses  itself, 
suddenly  denying  the  girls  the  very  rewards  it  has  held  out  to  them 
throughout  the  whole  course  of  their  development.  [40, 178] 

In  studying  college  girls'  attitudes  toward  their  role  in  the  post- 
war period,  Seward  found 

...  for  the  group  as  a  whole,  an  emphasis  on  equality  between 
men  and  women  in  educational  and  vocational  opportunities,  working 
conditions,  community  activities,  and  social  contacts.  Inconsistent 
with  this  liberal  trend  was  a  reactionary  reinforcement  of  the  tradi- 
tional subordinate  feminine  role  as  far  as  wife  and  mother  relation- 
ships were  concerned.  [40, 193] 

As  would  be  predicted,  those  subjects  who  rejected  the  latter  no- 
tions, were  found  to  be  more  insecure  than  those  who  conformed, 
this  apparently  being  "the  price  .  .  .  [they]  pay  for  their  non- 
conformity." [190]  Further  evidence  that  this  conflict  is  of  social, 
not  biological  origin,  was  found  in  interviews  in  which  the  "at- 
titudes and  experiences  with  respect  to  sexual  and  maternal  ac- 
tivities could  not  be  differentiated  from  those  of  the  conservatives." 
[193]  This  finding  would  seem  to  go  counter  to  the  psycho- 
analytic theory  that  "all  women  .  .  .  who  fail  to  find  complete 
satisfaction  in  the  role  assigned  them  by  contemporary  society" 
have  a  "masculinity  complex."  [177]  For  example,  we  would  ex- 
pect that  girls  who  handled  men's  jobs  during  the  war  would  find 
it  particularly  difficult  to  return  to  the  role  conventionally  pre- 
scribed for  women. 

The  major  conflicts  and  anxieties  experienced  by  girls  who  are 
themselves  in  a  state  of  transition  can  be  traced  back  to  the  incon- 
sistent conflicting  values  and  norms  in  society.  This  fact  was  dis- 


DEVELOPING  EGO  RENDERED  UNSTABLE  AND,  AT  TIMES,  CRITICAL     239 

cussed  by  Leta  Hollingworth  [20]  17  years  ago  and  has  been  more 
recently  pointed  out  by  Seward  [40]. 

The  changing  ego  and  adult-youth  conflict.  The  recasting  of 
some  major  ego-attitudes  and  the  formation  of  new  ego-attitudes 
in  relation  to  one's  body,  to  one's  own  and  to  the  opposite  sex, 
grown-ups,  education,  work  or  profession,  standing  in  the  world, 
and  so  on,  all  imply  a  change  in  the  ego.  This  change  does  not 
occur  overnight.  The  ego  reaches  its  relative  (adult)  stability 
only  after  a  period  of  time,  if  it  reaches  it  at  all.  Under  some 
difficult  circumstances  and  for  some  persons,  the  striving  for  rela- 
tive stability  is  considerably  prolonged.  For  example,  for  some 
old  maids  or  bachelors  frustrated  during  adolescence,  the  process 
may  continue  throughout  a  whole  lifetime. 

We  have  already  pointed  out  that  because  of  the  heightened 
affective  and  emotional  state  of  the  adolescent  due  to  significant 
glandular  changes  and  to  the  accelerated  growth  rate,  he  is  also 
in  a  state  of  flux  psychologically.  The  more  or  less  stable  ego- 
links  formed  up  to  adolescence  become  shaky  and  precarious. 
New  relationships,  links,  and  aspirations  are  still  in  a  fluid  state 
and  subject  to  "mercurial"  ups  and  downs  (to  use  Zachry's  char- 
acterization), on  account  of  the  physiological  flux  and  external 
(social)  pressures.  The  adolescent  tosses  about  and  suffers  from 
the  lack  of  stable  anchorages  until  some  new  ones  are  achieved. 
This  instability  is  especially  aggravated  in  a  changing  social  milieu 
and  in  different  family  situations  where  the  adolescent  faces  con- 
tradictory norms  and  inconsistent  expectations  and  treatments. 

The  psychological  consequences  are  feelings  of  inadequacy,  in- 
security, and  anxiety.  Various  degrees  of  crisis  may  ensue,  in 
some  cases  with  grave  consequences  for  the  individual.  The 
intensity  of  the  crisis  is  proportional  to  the  degree  of  friction  and 
contradiction  of  the  external  demands  and  individual  internal  fac- 
tors. The  adolescent  may  feel  all  alone  as  he  finds  himself  caught 
in  the  whirl  of  sex  desires,  striving  to  amount  to  something  in 
the  midst  of  contradictory  and  inconsistent  relationships  and  val- 
ues. Several  expressions  of  this  feeling  of  aloneness,  of  being  left 
out  and  even  betrayed,  were  found  in  the  diaries  of  Turkish  ado- 
lescent boys  and  girls.  The  writing  of  diaries,  which  indicates  a 
turning  inward,  seems  to  be  more  prevalent  in  a  social  milieu 


240  RE-FORMATION  OF  THE  EGO  IN  ADOLESCENCE 

which  does  not  provide  many  opportunities  for  the  adolescents  of 
both  sexes  to  mix  together  frequently  in  overt  activities  such  as 
age-mate  parties,  dancing  and  sports.  That  this  tendency  to  turn 
inward  is  reduced  in  a  social  milieu  where  adolescents  do  have 
more  of  a  chance  to  mix  together  is  indicated,  for  example,  by 
the  fact  that  at  the  present  time  in  the  upper  middle-class  milieu 
in  California  very  little  diary  writing  is  observed.  We  mention 
the  keeping  of  diaries  only  as  an  illustration  in  passing.  Before 
we  give  a  brief  account  of  other  behavioral  indications  of  the  ado- 
lescent's striving  to  amount  to  something  in  his  changed  relation- 
ships and  values,  his  effort  to  reanchor  himself,  we  must  examine 
somewhat  closely  the  implications  of  adult-youth  conflict  for  our 
problem. 

Adult-youth  conflict.  The  facts  reported  concerning  adult- 
youth  conflict  may  be  taken  as  a  significant  index  of  the  instabil- 
ity and,  in  many  cases  one  might  almost  say,  of  the  disintegration 
of  the  childish  ego.  Up  to  the  developmental  stage  of  puberty, 
a  more  or  less  stable  set  of  ego-attitudes  had  been  achieved  in  rela- 
tion to  parents,  other  grown-ups,  the  values  inculcated,  school 
authorities,  teachers,  and  age  mates.  With  the  changing  body  and 
changing  self,  with  the  changed  expectations  and  treatments  from 
the  social  surroundings  including  the  family,  even  these  existing 
relationships  may  be  altered.6 

Zachry  [49]  has  given  an  interesting  account  of  these  changes 
in  her  chapter  on  "Changing  Relationships  with  Adults."  For 
example,  the  adolescent  sees  himself  now  as  grown-up  or,  at 
least,  as  different,  whereas  the  family  at  times  still  continues  to 
look  at  him  or  to  treat  him  as  the  baby  child.  The  case  of  John 
Sanders  [25],  referred  to  earlier,  is  a  good  illustration  of  the  point. 
In  relation  to  John's  mother,  Jones  states: 

It  does  not  appear,  however,  that  she  could  regard  with  objective 
tolerance  the  preliminary  signs  of  John's  adolescent  rebellion;  to  her, 

6  This  does  not,  of  course,  mean  a  denial  of  the  special  cases  of  significant 
changes  in  parent-youth  relationships  at  an  earlier  age  in  unfortunate  family  situa- 
tions. In  such  cases,  age  mates  in  the  streets,  school,  or  in  gang  formations  may 
exert  a  dominant  role.  Also  we  are  not  ignoring  prematurely  produced  effects  in 
those  cases  where  economic  responsibilities  are  imposed  on  boys  and  girls  at  an 
early  age. 


DEVELOPING  EGO  RENDERED  UNSTABLE  AND,  AT  TIMES,  CRITICAL     241 

these  changing  attitudes  were  a  source  of  irritation  and  were  appar- 
ently not  thought  of  as  related  to  her  son's  social  maturing.  [25,  20] 

The  "gripes"  and  "peeves*'  adolescents  have  against  their  parents 
because  of  the  inconsistent  way  they  are  treated  are  reflected  in 
the  following  list  mentioned  by  a  16-year-old  boy: 

Being  called  in  the  morning  more  than  twice.  Having  them  tell 
me  what  to  eat.  Being  yelled  at  in  the  bathroom  in  the  morning. 
Being  asked  questions  about  homework.  Being  "called  down"  about 
my  school  marks.  Having  to  tell  them  where  I've  been  on  dates, 
where  I'm  going  and  who  I'm  going  with.  Always  being  nagged 
about  the  length  of  time  I  use  the  phone,  the  light  I  read  in,  and  the 
radio  programs  I  hear.7 

Goodenough  summarizes  this  source  of  parent-youth  conflict  as 
follows: 

The  conflict  between  old  habits  and  new  requirements — represented 
on  the  part  of  the  adolescent  by  his  feeling  that  he  is  grown  up,  that 
he  wants  to  be  treated  like  a  grown-up,  while  he  still  has  habits  of 
acting  like  a  child,  and  on  the  parents'  side  by  their  recognition  that 
the  child  is  growing  up,  their  feeling  that  he  ought  to  act  more  like  a 
grown-up,  although  from  force  of  habit  they  continue  to  treat  him  as 
if  he  were  still  a  child — often  makes  for  a  good  deal  of  friction. 
[17,  494] 

Parent-youth  conflict  is  the  more  general  formulation  of  the  facts 
covered  under  "psychological  weaning,"  or  the  tendency  toward 
independence  (emancipation)  on  the  part  of  adolescents.  As  a 
result  of  such  situations,  the  adolescent  boy  or  girl  makes  im- 
plicit or  explicit,  effective  or,  most  of  the  time,  futile  moves  to- 
ward emancipation  and  independence.  These  rebellions  may,  in 
some  cases,  assume  serious  proportions.  Running  away  from  home 
and  wanting  to  study  in  out-of-town  places,  are  examples.  Illus- 
trations of  this  parent-youth  conflict  have  been  reported  by  many 
observers.  We  can  choose  at  random  a  few  of  the  many  concrete 
cases. 
Among  other  cases,  Averill  [1]  relates  this  one: 

7  This  is  one  of  the  representative  interviews  collected  for  us  by  Carolyn  Bcrl. 


242  RE-FORMATION  OF  THE  EGO  IN  ADOLESCENCE 

Ruth,  a  sophomore  in  high  school,  is  known  to  her  classmates  as  a 
"goody-goody,"  an  appellation  which  she  greatly  resents.  At  home, 
she  indulges  in  the  wildest  temper-tantrums  to  get  her  own  way  from 
parents  who  have  scant  understanding  of  the  social  needs  of  a  young 
girl.  .  .  .  Her  clothes  are  neat,  but  practical  rather  than  particularly 
becoming.  To  Ruth  they  seem  ugly,  just  because  they  are  so  practical. 
She  begs  to  be  allowed  to  choose  her  own  clothing,  but  is  never 
permitted  any  voice  in  its  selection.  .  .  .  No  company  is  allowed  on 
school  nights,  and  no  excuse  is  sufficient  to  gain  for  Ruth  escape  from 
her  homework.  She  is  never  permitted  to  attend  the  school  parties, 
and  is  resentful  at  the  strict  prohibitions  which  keep  her  from  par- 
ticipating in  all  club  and  dramatic  activities.  At  school  she  is  accused 
of  lacking  school  spirit,  and  suffers  agonies  in  consequence.  Of  late, 
her  tantrums  have  been  getting  worse,  and  her  parents,  though  coun- 
seled to  ignore  them,  are  becoming  greatly  worried.  Frequently  she 
is  quite  ill  for  a  day  or  two  following  these  attacks,  and  insists  that 
she  has  no  interest  in  getting  well.  [1,  69] 

Zachry  [49]  reports  a  case  which  is  interesting  because  of  the 
indication  it  gives  of  an  adolescent's  awareness  of  the  nature  of 
the  adult-youth  conflict: 

A  high-school  senior  whose  home  was  pervaded  by  an  atmosphere 
of  artificial  politeness,  in  which  it  was  almost  unthinkable  to  express 
resentment  openly,  wrote  in  a  theme  entitled  "The  Everburning  Fire 
of  Youth": 

"When  the  boy  and  girl  first  begin  to  take  notice  of  a  current 
problem  they  see  the  cause  of  the  problem  with  a  clearer  and  cleaner 
sense  of  view  than  do  most  of  their  elders,  who  are  too  burdened  with 
many  worldly  ideas.  .  .  . 

"It  is  disappointing  to  the  younger  set  to  find  that  their  elders  do 
not  want  to  cooperate.  Many  times  when  a  young  boy  or  girl  tries 
to  give  some  advice  he  is  laughed  out  of  the  picture  by  his  elders, 
who  feel  that  youth  has  no  place  in  the  world  except  to  listen  and 
learn.  .  .  . 

"As  the  boy  and  girl  grow  older  they  are  given  more  chance  to 
have  their  say,  but  by  this  time  the  majority  have  either  forgotten 
their  ideals  or  else  the  events  have  become  so  muddled  that  there  is 
no  chance  of  clear  youthful  thinking."  [49,  311] 

A  case  of  conflict  in  an  adolescent  boy  of  an  average  New  Eng- 
land family  is  taken  from  Healy  [19]  "out  of  the  hundreds  of 


DEVELOPING  EGO  RENDERED  UNSTABLE  AND,  AT  TIMES,  CRITICAL     243 

available  illustrations  of  such  conflicts  in  our  records."  [141]  Both 
the  father  and  mother  are  average  normal  New  England  folks. 
There  are  no  unusual  disturbing  frictions  in  the  situation  up  to 
the  time  of  adolescence. 

The  issues  came  out  so  clearly  because  he  began  to  rebel  at  their 
formulae  of  life  by  the  time  he  was  twelve  years  old.  The  stirrings  of 
an  unusually  fine  physique  and  a  very  early  adolescence,  together 
with  a  violent  rejection  of  an  earlier  father  identification,  led  him  to 
phantasy  hazardous  adventure  and  to  seek  out  young  daredevils  be- 
longing to  the  unacceptable  fringe  of  his  neighborhood  with  whom 
he  engaged  in  reckless  escapades  and  delinquencies.  Undetected  in 
the  latter  he  then,  to  the  utter  astonishment  of  his  family,  began  a 
series  of  runaway  episodes.  "I  can't  stand  it,  doing  everything  just 
the  way  they  want  me  to.  They  are  good  enough  to  me,  but  I  can't 
talk  to  them  about  the  way  I  feel,"  this  wholesome  appearing  lad  told 
us  when  we  saw  him  after  he  had  been  so  perplexing  his  family  for 
a  couple  of  years.  [19, 142  f.] 

Adult-youth  conflict  is  such  a  general  phenomenon  in  modern 
complex  societies  that  the  problem  does  not  have  to  be  subjected 
to  a  precise  psychological  scrutiny  to  tap  its  existence.  It  occa- 
sionally occupies  the  columns  of  daily  papers.  Sometimes,  these 
cases  are  serious  indeed;  but  here  we  will  note  a  comparatively 
minor  manifestation.  For  example,  a  pretty  15-year-old  girl  re- 
cently remarked  to  a  newspaperman: 

Teachers  and  parents  make  all  the  plans,  and  we  run  all  the 
errands.  We're  always  on  the  defensive.  Grownups  don't  trust  us 
or  give  us  any  responsibilities.  Take  our  city.  We're  working  like 
dogs  to  get  a  youth  canteen.  We're  the  ones  who'll  use  it,  but  do  we 
have  anything  to  say  about  it?  Like  heck  we  do.  [36] 

Adult-youth  conflict  may  be  a  good  deal  more  than  the  mere 
individual  conflicts  of  individual  adolescents  of  certain  families. 
It  may  develop  into  a  standardized  attitude  among  adolescents. 
Such  a  standardized  adolescent  attitude  was  observed  by  Stolz, 
M.  C.  Jones,  and  Chaff ey  [41]  in  one  of  the  many  adolescent 
studies  of  the  California  Institute  of  Child  Welfare.  From  their 
investigation  of  "The  Junior  High  School  Age,"  "based  upon  an 


244  RE-FORMATION  OF  THE  EGO  IN  ADOLESCENCE 

intensive  study  of  one  hundred  boys  and  one  Hundred  girls  which 
has  been  carried  on  during  the  three-year  period  that  they  were 
enrolled  in  junior  high  school/'  the  authors  conclude: 

Adult  approval  or  disapproval  meant  almost  nothing  to  these  young 
adolescents  except  as  it  might  affect  the  attainment  of  their  goal.  In 
fact  there  was  a  noticeable  resistance,  not  so  much  to  authority,  as 
our  rules  seldom  got  in  their  way,  but  simply  to  adults  as  such.  Those 
girls  and  boys  who  were  in  the  throes  of  establishing  themselves 
socially  were  the  most  antagonistic  toward  adults.  They  manifested 
this  attitude  chiefly  by  shunning  adults  and  acting  as  if  their  presence 
were  a  hindrance.  Six  months  later  these  same  pupils  were  likely  to 
be  the  ones  who  hung  around  and  talked  to  adults  as  if,  being  quite 
grown  up  now,  they  needed  to  talk  and  associate  with  other  grown 
persons.  [41,  3] 

In  another  California  study  of  social  development  in  adoles- 
cence, Cameron  [10]  states: 

But  through  it  all  there  appears  the  steadily  rising  pressure  to 
break  away  from  the  earlier  accepted  domination  of  parents  in  the 
home  and  of  teachers  in  the  school.  It  is  as  though  an  overwhelming 
urge  were  released  within  many  of  these  youngsters  to  assert  their 
independence,  to  explore  quite  new  and  thrilling  kinds  of  relationships 
with  each  other,  and  to  proclaim  their  rights  to  self-expression  as  in- 
dividuals. In  a  sense  the  clubhouse  was  misnamed,  for  no  club  meet- 
ings were  held  in  it.  Nor  has  any  desire  been  expressed  for  the 
formation  of  clubs.  The  social  organization  which  these  adolescents 
prefer  is  less  rigid,  more  changeable  and  fluid.  Formalities  are  for- 
gotten in  their  quest  for  more  personal  relationships.  [10,  4] 

In  a  country  in  transition,  in  which  the  discrepancy  between  the 
generations  is  that  of  different  social  periods,  adult-youth  conflict 
may  acquire  still  greater  proportions.  In  Turkey,  which  has  been 
going  through  such  a  stage  of  transition,  we  have  collected  ma- 
terial since  1937  (through  diaries,  interviews,  and  questionnaires) 
on  the  social  psychology  of  adolescence  with  special  emphasis  on 
the  problems  of  attitudes  and  ego  formation.  Among  other  ma- 
terial collected  were  the  reactions  of  over  3,000  boys  and  girls  in 
different  parts  of  the  country  to  a  questionnaire  especially  designed 
to  tap  these  problems.  The  reactions  were  obtained  under  con- 


DEVELOPING  EGO  RENDERED  UNSTABLE  AND,  AT  TIMES,  CRITICAL     245 

trolled  conditions.8  We  summarize  here  only  the  main  results 
relating  to  adult-youth  conflict.  Two  of  the  questions  were  espe- 
cially relevant  to  the  present  discussion:  "Do  you  think  grown- 
ups understand  you  as  you  are?"  and  "If  there  are  persons  who 
understand  you  as  you  are,  who  are  they?" 

The  principal  finding  was  that  younger  boys  and  girls  (approxi- 
mately between  the  ages  11  and  14)  report,  in  general,  that  par- 
ents, relatives,  or  teachers  understand  them.  But  above  this  age 
and  in  late  adolescence,  in  general  the  report  is  that  grown-ups 
do  not  understand  them.  Many  boys  and  girls  said  that  only  a 
few  of  their  friends,  who  are  their  age  mates,  understand  them 
as  they  are.  Some  reported  that  nobody  understands  them,  in- 
cluding themselves.  Among  the  introspections  they  volunteered 
to  write  down,  many  reveal  the  important  issues  of  transition. 
We  reproduce  here  as  illustrations  only  two  of  these  introspective 
reports. 

The  grown-ups  cannot  understand  me,  because  they  are  people  of 
the  last  century  whereas  I  belong  to  this  century.  Things  which  do 
not  please  them  are  very  pleasing  to  me. 

Another  girl  wrote: 

We  do  not  think  grown-ups  understand  us.  As  there  is  a  great 
difference  between  the  periods  in  which  we  have  grown  up  they  mis- 
understand us  and  frictions  come  out  as  a  consequence.  [35] 

These  findings  hold  only  for  town  and  city  adolescents  attending 
school.  The  problems  of  adolescents  in  rural  districts  present  dif- 
ferent characteristics  since,  until  recent  years,  very  few  rural  chil- 
dren in  Turkey  attended  school  and  the  great  majority  of  them 
had  to  participate  at  an  early  age  in  economic  life.  These  quali- 
fying remarks  are  made  to  call  attention  once  more  to  the  fact 
that  the  psychological  problems  (as  well  as  other  problems)  as- 
sume different  features  in  countries  in  transition,  according  to  the 
tempo  of  change  in  different  strata  and  regions  of  the  population. 
Perhaps  the  various  acculturation  studies  now  going  on  may  fur- 
nish valuable  data  for  dealing  with  the  psychology  of  these 
problems. 

8  The  results  of  these  studies  will  be  published  later. 


246  RE-FORMATION  OF  THE  EGO  IN  ADOLESCENCE 

It  may  be  said,  in  general,  that  parent-youth  conflict  increases 
with  the  rate  of  social  change.  [15]  The  Lynds  [33]  observed 
this  trend  in  Middletown. 

To  quote  a  veteran  worker  with  Middletown's  children,  "Our  par- 
ents are  realizing  the  increasingly  sharp  divergence  of  their  world  and 
that  of  their  children  today  as  never  before."  And  the  parents'  world 
strikes  back!  In  many  cases  they  attempt  to  use  the  schools  as  a  means 
of  holding  the  two  worlds  together.  A  high-school  course  in  sociology 
has  been  dropped  because  of  parental  protest  over  the  fact  that  prob- 
lems of  sex  were  discussed  in  class.  Over  the  heads  of  Middletown 
teachers,  trained  according  to  standards  wider  than  some  of  the  mores 
of  Middletown,  hangs  at  all  times  the  sword  of  parental  conservatism 
and  anxiety.  This  is  rendered  the  more  difficult  because,  in  manners 
and  morals  as  well  as  economics,  politics,  and  religion,  the  local  com- 
munity contains  taxpaying  parents  of  widely  varying  personal  stand- 
ards. The  teacher  knows  and  the  community  knows  that  the  children 
ranged  in  their  seats  are  wise  in  matters  not  in  the  curriculum,  and 
that  many  of  these  children  are  rebelliously  clamoring  for  the  right  to 
raise  questions  and  to  be  outspoken  in  the  face  of  the  official  and 
parental  restraints.  As  one  teacher  said,  "I  am  facing  a  new  problem 
nowadays:  My  pupils  insist  on  raising  questions  I  dare  not  let  them 
discuss  though  my  conscience  demands  that  I  not  clamp  down  op 
their  honest  questions.  The  things  they  say  continually  keep  me  on 
pins  and  needles  for  fear  some  of  them  will  go  home  and  tell  their 
parents.  I  have  an  uneasy  furtive  sense  about  it  all."  [33,  233] 

All  of  these  observations  indicate  that  new  generations  growing 
up  in  rapidly  changing  social  surroundings  resent,  and  at  times 
rebel  against,  the  efforts  of  grown-ups  to  shape  them  after  their 
own  images.  However,  the  impact  of  these  facts  concerning  adult- 
youth  conflict  must  be  qualified  when  viewed  from  the  perspec- 
tive secured  through  the  studies  of  ethnologists  and  sociologists. 
Adult-youth  conflict  reflects  in  a  significant  way  the  conflicts  be- 
tween the  established  generation  and  the  younger  generation.  The 
intensity  of  the  conflict  might  be  said  to  be,  by  and  large,  propor- 
tional to  the  degree  of  social  change.  In  a  society  in  which  the 
survival  norms  constitute  the  major  values  of  the  superstructure 
(as  the  Lynds  pointed  out)  and  lag  behind  the  material-techno- 
logical developments  ("cultural  lag"),  no  matter  how  serious  the 


DEVELOPING  EGO  RENDERED  UNSTABLE  AND,  AT  TIMES,  CRITICAL     247 

youthful  rebellion  and  restlessness  may  be,  adult-youth  conflict 
will,  in  most  cases,  be  an  intrafamily  and  intracommunity  affair. 
A  concrete  case,  personally  communicated,  illustrates  the  point. 
An  adolescent  boy  was  strongly  and,  at  times,  openly  critical  of 
everything  that  his  parents  (both  parents)  did.  Nevertheless,  he 
shared  the  major  class  delineations,  political  views,  and  social- 
distance  norms  of  his  upper-middle  class  parents. 

The  impact  of  special  socioeconomic  events  such  as  depression, 
the  introduction  of  new  industrial  developments  in  a  society  or 
even  in  a  particular  locality,  may  produce  new  problems  in  adult- 
youth  relationships.  For  example,  in  her  study  of  The  Unem- 
ployed Man  and  His  Family,  carried  on  in  1935-36,  Komarovsky 
[29]  presents  data  amply  illustrating  this  point.  One  important 
result  of  unemployment  was  to  undermine  "the  authority  of  the 
father  over  the  adolescent  child  even  more  frequently"  than  his 
authority  over  younger  children  or  his  authority  as  a  husband.  [92] 
This  deterioration  of  authority  came  chiefly  because  money  "was 
frequently  used  by  the  parents  as  an  instrument  of  education  and 
control."  [92]  In  the  light  of  our  preceding  discussion  of  the 
strivings  of  the  adolescent  to  amount  to  something  as  a  person  in 
his  own  right,  the  meaning  of  such  facts  becomes  apparent.  Also 
important  was  the  finding  that  intrafamily  and  parental  conflict 
increased  because  "father  is  a  changed  man"  since  he  lost  his 
work.  [94  ff.]  Furthermore,  conflict  between  father  and  adoles- 
cent children  was  found  to  be  particularly  intense  when  one  of  the 
children  was  working.  [97-102] 

Such  changes  in  the  established  parent-youth  relationships  due 
to  the  impact  of  serious  socioeconomic  events  suggest  that  the 
situational  determinations  of  parent-offspring  relationships  may  be 
more  significant  at  times  than  the  toilet  habits  or  postulated 
"Oedipus  complexes"  of  early  infancy.  At  least,  in  some  cases  of 
serious  economic  misery  or  serious  status  problems,  qualitative 
changes  in  intrafamily  relationships  may  be  brought  about  which 
go  beyond  the  limits  of  such  wholesale  formulations  as  those 
positing  immutable  childhood  complexes,  where,  we  are  told,  ad- 
verse social  circumstances  act  only  as  agents  which  release  nothing 
more  than  deep-rooted  jealousies,  hatreds,  and  loves  in  the  rela- 
tively fixed  storehouse  of  the  unconscious,  (ch.  14) 


248  RE-FORMATION  OF  THE  EGO  IN  ADOLESCENCE 

THE  ADOLESCENTS'  REACTIONS  AND  EFFORTS  TO  RE-ESTABLISH 
THEMSELVES  TO  SOME  DEGREE  OF  STABILITY 

In  our  sketchy  account  up  to  this  point  we  have  indicated  how 
the  adolescent's  ego  undergoes  significant  changes  with  his  chang- 
ing body,  his  altered  relationships  with  grown-ups  and  age  mates; 
and  how  varying  socioeconomic  circumstances  bring  about  cor- 
responding variations  that  produce  feelings  of  insecurity  and 
aloneness  and  different  degrees  and  durations  of  instability  in 
the  shaky  ego-attitudes.  We  saw  how,  as  a  consequence,  strivings 
for  independence  and  emancipation  arise.  These  lead  to  the  ado- 
lescent's "psychological  weaning"  from  grown-ups,  and  this  in 
turn  produces  varying  degrees  and  kinds  of  adult-youth  conflict. 

Thus,  having  lost  a  more  or  less  stabilized  ground  of  ego  links, 
caught  in  the  .whirl  of  new  sex  and  other  desires  which  meet 
various  degrees  of  resistances  from  social  surroundings  to  which 
he  has  to  adjust  himself  anew,  the  adolescent  is  torn  away  from 
his  more  or  less  solid  earlier  ego  links.  In  the  throes  of  re-estab- 
lishing himself  as  a  person  in  his  own  right,  he  may,  consciously 
or  unconsciously,  resort  to  innumerable  reactions  in  addition  to  his 
striving  to  satisfy  his  strengthened  and  specified  desires.  The  ego 
and  ego  links  which  have  become  more  unstable  and  insecure  must 
be  re-established,  at  least  to  some  degree  of  stability  with  the  new 
grown-up  values  whatever  they  may  be  to  him.  Being  "in  a 
vacuum,"  being  marginal  in  so  many  diverse  relationships  of  a 
differentiated  society  is  painful,  especially  during  a  transition  stage 
when  the  adolescent  is  so  keenly  aware  of  these  ego  issues  in  rela- 
tion to  his  body,  his  male  and  female  friends,  his  parents,  and  so 
forth. 

In  studies  of  adolescence,  hundreds  of  different  kinds  of  reac- 
tions are  reported  which  reveal  the  painful  and  critical  process  the 
adolescent  goes  through  in  re-establishing  himself.  We  shall  give 
some  samples  of  these  reactions  and  then  return  to  our  problem 
more  specifically.  As  Hollingworth  [20]  points  out,  some  adoles- 
cents demand  and  feel  thwarted  if  they  are  refused  privacy,  their 
"own"  room.  [187  f.]  As  already  mentioned,  some  adolescents, 
particularly  girls,  keep  diaries.  Hollingworth  [21,  595]  and 


ADOLESCENTS'  EFFORTS  TO  RE-ESTABLISH  THEMSELVES  249 

Biihler  [8,  390]  both  cite  studies  indicating  that  diaries  are  fre- 
quently the  recipients  of  the  adolescent's  most  secret  desires,  hopes, 
and  dreams.  In  Turkey  where,  as  we  noted;  diaries  are  more 
common  than  in  countries  where  adolescents  have  more  oppor- 
tunity for  extracurricular  activities  providing  for  ever-enlarging 
group  activities,  one  girl  of  17  wrote  in  her  diary: 

Oh,  my  beloved  diary,  I  return  to  you!    You  alone  understand  me. 
I  feel  so  lost  and  alone  in  this  cruel  world.  [35] 

An  adolescent  boy  or  girl  may  become  absorbed  in  daydreams  or 
phantasies  in  which,  usually,  he  or  she  is  the  central  character.  [12, 
20]  Many  adolescents  are  absorbed  in  the  unreal  world  of  Holly- 
wood movies,  the  idolizing  of  movie  stars.  If  there  is  one  Mecca 
for  bourgeois  youth  all  over  the  world  today,  it  seems  to  be 
Hollywood. 

Sometimes,  if  conflict  is  too  great,  if  failure  to  re-establish  oneself 
occurs  for  too  long  or  too  often,  abnormal  behavior  may  result. 
Thus  Ellen  Hill,  who  in  Davis  and  Dollard's  Children  of  Bond- 
age [14]  was  described  by  relatives,  teachers,  and  friends  as  having 
been,  as  a  child,  "an  especially  agreeable  girl,  'sweet'  in  her  dis- 
position," "a  favorite  with  her  instructors,"  "very  tractable,"  be- 
came, during  early  adolescence,  "impudent,"  "malicious,"  "sly," 
"very  belligerent,"  and  finally  "definitely  abnormal."  [158  ff.] 
She  talked  to  teachers  only  in  monosyllables.  She  dreamed  day 
and  night  of  deprivations  or  alleviations  of  her  situation,  both 
material  and  social.  Ellen's  case  is  not  a  simple  one.  However, 
the  authors  make  it  clear  that  this  crisis  came  as  a  result  of  an 
intense  impact  of  economic  and  family  difficulties  which  increased 
suddenly  in  her  early  adolescence  and  seriously  threatened  her 
standing  with  her  clique.  The  combination  of  factors  associated 
with  adolescence,  her  aspirations  to  rise  even  higher  in  status,  and 
the  crushing  realities  of  economic  deprivation  and  disgrace  in  her 
family  brought  on  these  behavioral  symptoms  which  the  authors 
characterize  as  "status  anxiety  .  .  ,  which,  in  the  lives  of  persons 
who  have  experienced  rapid  change  in  status,  either  upward  or 
downward,  may  be  viewed  as  a  form  of  'status  shock.' "  [156] 

Another  example  of  more  or  less  abnormal  behavior  is  that  of 
adolescents  who  are  frequently  ill,  with  no  discoverable  organic 


250  RE-FORMATION  OF  THE  EGO  IN  ADOLESCENCE 

disorders.  Several  authors  have  mentioned  the  relative  frequency 
of  the  onset  of  schizophrenia,  manic-depressive  psychoses,  and 
hysteria  during  adolescence.  [12,  20,  39,  47]  Suicide,  which  is 
somewhat  more  common  in  complex  and  transitional  societies, 
may  be  resorted  to.  Hollingworth  [20]  aptly  described  adolescent 
suicide  when  she  wrote: 

The  adolescent  prefers  death  to  the  torture  of  his  uncertainties  and 
thwartings,  and  takes  active  steps  to  destroy  himself.  .  .  .  The  self  is 
lost  irretrievably.  .  .  .  [20,  799  /.,  italics  ours] 

Among  the  many  illustrations  of  adolescents'  attempts  to  re- 
establish themselves,  Hollingworth  cites  that  of  a  girl  who  read 
the  Bible  and  prayed  in  the  morning  and  rode  horses  bareback  all 
afternoon,  much  to  her  family's  consternation.  The  girl,  it  de- 
veloped, was  trying  to  decide  between  the  occupations  of  dea- 
coness and  bareback  rider.  [171  /.]  This  illustration  gives  us  some 
clue  to  Conklin's  interesting  study  [12]  of  329  college  students 
who  were  asked  if  they  had  ever  experienced  more  than  one  "self 
at  the  same  time.  Twenty-six  per  cent  could  immediately  recall 
having  experienced  two  fairly  different  "selves"  at  the  same  time. 
"Twenty-eight  per  cent  of  the  above  group  reported  that  they  had 
observed  the  same  phenomenon  in  others."  [143] 

Zachry  [49]  mentions  the  case  of  a  girl  who  threw  herself  into 
sports  activities  and  determined  to  attain  perfection  because  she 
felt  that  the  female  body  was  physically  inadequate.  Two  adoles- 
cent sisters  who  had  no  friends  among  their  age  mates  engaged 
in  work  and  study  as  a  substitute  activity  to  the  point  of  overwork- 
ing themselves.  Another  girl  became  "over-assertive,"  pushing 
herself  to  the  fore  in  social  gatherings.  Still  other  cases  are  re- 
ported who  became  self-deprecatory,  cynical,  or  weighed  down 
with  self-blame  and  guilt. 

In  the  struggle  to  re-establish  themselves  anew,  adolescents  may 
choose  idols,  or  certain  characteristics  of  several  persons,  and  strive 
to  emulate  them.  Sometimes  the  model  may  be  a  movie  star, 
public  figure,  fictional  character  or  teacher.  One  adolescent  tried 
to  imitate  her  teacher  in  every  way,  saw  no  faults  in  her,  and 
allowed  no  one  to  speak  ill  of  her.  [20,  182]  Crushes  are  also 
found  between  age  mates  of  the  same  sex,  particularly  among  girls. 


THE  EFFECTS  OF  AGE-MATE  REFERENCE  GROUPS  251 

It  is  not  difficult  to  find  two  adolescent  girls  who  spend  most  of 
their  time  together,  dress  similarly,  act  very  much  alike,  protect 
each  other,  and,  in  general,  identify  with  each  other,  at  least  for  a 
time.  Cases  of  "puppy  love"  are  epidemic  in  adolescence.  As  we 
noted,  adolescents  may,  under  intense  stress,  undertake  the  serious 
business  of  marriage  at  an  early  age  as  a  way  out  of  their  conflicts. 
Some  adolescent  loves  may,  indeed,  be  both  intense  and  lasting. 
However,  many  are  highly  temporary  and  frequent.  Since  love 
can  be  described  as  a  high  degree  of  ego-involvement  with  another 
person  in  addition  to  sex  attraction  and  desires,  this  rapid  shifting 
and  changing  of  adolescent  loves  is  an  understandable  result  of 
the  mercurial  state  of  the  adolescent's  ego. 

THE  EFFECTS  OF  AGE-MATE  REFERENCE  GROUPS  AND  MEMBERSHIP 
GROUPS  IN  THE  RE-FORMATION  OF  THE  EGO-ATTITUDES 

The  developing  and  increasing  adolescent  attitudes  and  interests, 
itemized  in  detail  in  so  many  works  on  the  subject,  gain  in  psy- 
chological meaning  and  coherence  if  they  are  studied  in  terms  of 
the  reference  groups  and  membership  groups  to  which  the  adoles- 
cent relates  himself.  For  many  of  these  adolescent  attitudes  and 
interests  develop  in  interaction  with  and  conformity  to  such 
groups.  The  adolescents'  most  intense  strivings  for  status  and 
approval  take  place  within  such  groups.  Here  our  main  concern 
is,  of  course,  with  ego-attitudes. 

Adolescents  progressively  turn  to  the  closer  company  of  age 
mates  in  their  transition  from  childhood  to  adulthood,  in  their 
struggle  to  establish  themselves  as  persons  in  their  own  rights  in 
an  adverse  adult-made  world  in  which  they  are  marginal  in  vary- 
ing degrees.  They  interact  in  their  own  adolescent  circles,  limited 
and  influenced,  of  course,  by  their  particular  social  setting  at  large. 
This  gives  rise  to  certain  norms  of  behavior,  to  fashions  and  fads 
of  dress  and  amusement  peculiar  to  various  adolescent  groups. 
During  these  years  of  transition,  adolescents  achieve  immediate 
status  through  conformity  to  the  norms  of  their  age-mate  groups. 
For  the  time  being  these  peculiar  adolescent  norms  of  experience 
and  behavior  become  the  adolescent's  own  values,  determining  his 
personal  relationships  and  attitudes  to  an  important  degree.  It  is 


252  RE-FORMATION  OF  THE  EGO  IN  ADOLESCENCE 

not  a  mere  accident  of  phraseology,  therefore,  that  Harold  Jones 
and  other  authorities  in  the  field  refer  to  the  ensemble  of  these 
adolescent  standardizations  as  "adolescent  culture."  These  adoles- 
cent norms  may  be  of  various  durations,  some  may  even  be  sea- 
sonal affairs.  They  may  and  certainly  do  change  from  society  to 
society,  even  in  different  regions  or  districts  of  the  same  society. 
But  as  long  as  they  last,  they  demand  conformity  from  the  adoles- 
cents within  the  particular  reference  group  to  which  they  apply. 
The  adolescent  derives  his  status,  with  the  age  mates  among  whom 
he  moves  to  some  extent  at  least,  from  the  fulfillment  of  expecta- 
tions prescribed  by  them.  In  this  connection,  we  can  advance  the 
hypothesis  that  the  degree  of  influence  of  age-mate  reference 
groups  and  membership  groups  varies  directly  with  the  degree  of 
psychological  weaning  from  grown-ups  and  the  intensity  of  adult- 
youth  conflict. 

The  effect  of  age-mate  reference  groups  on  ego-attitudes.  As  we 
noted,  probably  proportional  to  the  degree  of  psychological  wean- 
ing from  grown-ups  and  strivings  for  independence,  adolescents 
seek  in  an  increasing  way  the  company  of  their  age  mates  or  peers. 
The  influence  of  companions  in  determining  the  adolescents'  likes- 
dislikes,  interests  and  attitudes,  increases  correspondingly.  His 
status  or  ego-values  are  increasingly  derived  from  age-mate  asso- 
ciations. These  adolescent  values  change,  of  course,  from  society 
to  society  and  vary  even  according  to  the  particular  socioeconomic 
standing  of  the  immediate  milieu.  We  designate  the  age-mate 
groups  to  which  adolescent  relates  himself  as  his  reference  group. 

Authorities  in  the  field  of  adolescence,  such  as  Hollingworth, 
Harold  Jones,  Zachry,  Bios,  Goodenough,  and  others  all  give 
interesting  hints  of  the  increased  influence  of  age  mates  during 
adolescence.  Zachry  [49]  remarks: 

In  the  desire  to  be  liked  by  his  own  group,  the  adolescent  usually 
does  his  best  to  conform  to  its  standards,  even  at  considerable  cost  to 
himself.  [49,  355] 

Consequently,  while  adamant  to  the  criticisms  and  advice  of 
parents,  the  adolescent  may  be  more  receptive  to  the  advice  of 
age-mate  groups.  This  is  well  illustrated  by  Zachry's  concrete 
observation; 


THE  EFFECTS  OF  AGE-MATE  REFERENCE  GROUPS  253 

A  sixteen-year-old  girl  was  saying  she  thought  she  could  take  criti- 
cism from  friends  which  she  would  not  accept  from  her  family.  She 
related  that  last  year  she  wore  bangs.  Her  mother  was  not  sure  she 
liked  them,  but  the  student  kept  the  bangs  because  her  girl  friends  all 
said  they  looked  very  well.  The  only  negative  criticism  friends  have 
given  her  is  that  she  is  too  loud.  "The  family  have  been  telling  me 
for  years  the  same  thing,"  she  explained.  "  'That's  enough  noise  from 
you,'  they  say.  Lately  I've  been  quiet."  [49,  355] 

On  the  basis  of  such  observations  Zachry  concisely  formulates 
the  regulation  of  the  adolescent's  values  by  his  reference  group: 

In  the  struggle  to  establish  himself  as  a  person  in  his  own  right, 
independent  of  adults,  the  adolescent  measures  his  success  against 
that  of  those  whose  status  is  similar  to  his.  The  greater  success  of 
some  of  them  in  one  aspect  or  another  of  development  seems  to 
threaten  to  impair  the  solidarity  of  those  on  whom  he  depends.  Also 
it  may  appear  to  him  a  direct  challenge  to  his  adequacy.  Differences 
in  degree  of  success  in  achieving  standards  that  are  important  to  peers 
(and  some  of  those  that  are  prized  by  parents  and  teachers  as  well) 
are  therefore  elements  in  their  emotional  relationships.  Economic 
status,  social  conventionality,  ethical  standards,  religious  and  political 
beliefs,  academic  success,  athletic  prowess,  ability  to  win  the  indulgence 
of  adults,  or  popularity  with  members  of  the  other  sex — any  one  of 
these  or  all  together— as  he  and  his  peers  embody  them  are  to  the 
adolescent  measures  of  his  own  success  in  establishing  himself  in 
growing  independence.  [49,  369  f.] 

Likewise  Bios  [5]  has  given  a  concise  summary  of  the  impact  of 
"peer-culture"  in  shaping  the  adolescent's  personal  values  of  failure 
or  success: 

Group  opinion  serves,  then,  as  a  selective  influence  for  desirable  and 
undesirable  behavior,  and  the  approval  or  disapproval  of  peers  be- 
comes progressively  the  most  influential  force  in  motivating  adolescent 
conduct.  [5,  249] 

And 

This  belongingness  to  the  group,  which  becomes  progressively  im- 
portant for  the  adolescent,  replaces  family  ties  to  some  extent  and 
thus  prepares  him  for  new  conformities  and  identifications  implicit 
in  the  group  life  of  adults.  [5,  250] 


254  RE-FORMATION  OF  THE  EGO  IN  ADOLESCENCE 

Bios  then  goes  on  to  indicate  that  the  attitudes  and  behavior  of 
the  individual  cannot  be  understood  properly  without  reference  to 
his  age-mate  groups: 

The  intermediary  phase  of  social  development  which  takes  place  at 
adolescence  can  be  properly  evaluated  only  with  reference  to  its  in- 
trinsic functions  and  meanings.  One  of  its  most  unique  functions  is 
to  establish  a  group  life  with  its  own  standards,  values,  appreciations. 
This  group  life,  often  referred  to  as  peer  culture,  has  a  decisive  impact 
upon  the  adolescent's  development  and  is  indeed  far  more  influential 
at  times  than  adult  opinion  or  judgment.  In  fact  adults  are  often 
unable  to  comprehend  the  peculiar  logic  of  adolescent  behavior  that 
is  perfectly  reasonable  to  adolescents  themselves.  A  girl  of  14  ex* 
pressed  herself  on  this  matter  and  said  that  sometimes  she  felt  that 
boys  and  girls  of  her  age  understand  each  other  better  than  their 
parents  understand  them.  For  instance,  she  continued,  "They'll  do 
some  silly  thing,  and  the  parents  won't  understand  why  it  is  they're 
so  silly "  [5,  251] 

GoodcnoughV  account  [17]  is  in  harmony  with  these: 

Unquestionably  the  influence  of  his  associates  upon  the  way  the 
adolescent  thinks  and  acts  is  very  great;  greater,  probably  than  at  any 
previous  stage  of  his  life.  For  the  adolescent  there  can  be  no  stronger 
argument  for  having  or  doing  a  thing  than  the  fact  that  "all  the  others 
are  doing  it."  Nothing  is  likely  to  awaken  so  great  an  emotional 
disturbance  or  cause  so  much  worry  as  the  feeling  that  he  is  in  some 
way  different  from  the  others.  "Others"  in  this  case,  means  the  other 
members  of  his  own  particular  group;  he  is  not  especially  concerned 
about  resembling  those  belonging  to  some  other  clan.  A  fashion 
started  by  the  leaders  of  a  group,  even  though  it  may  happen  to  be 
uncomfortable  or  inconvenient,  is  faithfully  copied  by  all  the  lesser 
members.  Opinions,  prejudices,  beliefs,  UJ(es,  and  dislikes  are  likewise 
determined  by  the  group,  and  the  boy  or  girl  who  differs  is  made  to 
feel  the  force  of  group  ostracism  unless  he  has  sufficient  force  of  per* 
sonality  to  bring  the  others  around  to  his  point  of  view.  [17,  492  /., 
italics  ours] 

Such  accounts  of  adolescent  groups  clarify  the  observations  made 
earlier,  such  as  those  of  John  Sanders  (p.  231)  concerning  discrep- 
ancies in  physical  and,  consequently,  in  social  maturity  as  com- 


THE  EFFECTS  OF  AGE-MATE  REFERENCE  GROUPS  255 

pared  with  age  mates  and  the  resulting  group  discrimination 
against  the  deviant  boy  or  girl. 

It  is  not  difficult  to  compile  detailed  items  of  values  concerning 
popularity  and  friendship,  fads  and  fashions,  and  other  standards 
that  prevail  among  adolescent  groups  and  that  must  be  conformed 
to  if  an  age  mate  uses  such  a  group  as  his  reference  group.  For 
example,  Averill  [1]  gives  a  two  and  a  half  page  list  of  fads  pre- 
vailing among  high  school  pupils  in  1935.  [208-211]  Because  of 
space  considerations  we  cannot,  unfortunately,  reproduce  such  lists 
here.  The  regulation  of  adolescent  behavior  in  relation  to  age- 
mate  groups  was  further  verified  in  a  survey  made  by  Life  maga- 
zine, [30]  The  reporter  prefaced  his  pictures  of  teen-age  girls 
with  this  revealing  observation: 

It  [the  adolescent  world]  is  also  a  world  of  many  laws.  They  are 
capricious  laws,  changing  or  reversing  themselves  almost  overnight. 
But  while  they  are  in  effect,  the  laws  are  immutable  and  the  punish- 
ment for  violation  is  ostracism,  swift  and  terrifying  practise  of  ancient 
peoples.  Months  ago  colored  bobby  sox  folded  at  the  top  were  de- 
creed, not  by  anyone  or  any  group,  but,  as  usual,  by  a  sudden  mysteri- 
ous and  universal  acceptance  of  the  new  idea.  Now  no  teen-ager 
dares  wear  anything  but  pure  white  sox  without  a  fold.  [30,  91] 

Such  observations  are  not  confined  to  adolescent  girls.    Another 
Life  survey  [32]  of  teen-age  boys  gives  a  similar  picture: 

Wherever  they  are  seen,  teen-age  boys  have  a  comfortable,  sloppy 
look.  Their  sloppiness  is  not  haphazard  but  is  governed  by  definite 
though  changing  sets  of  fashions.  The  current  style  for  daily  wear 
as  evidenced  by  the  boys  of  Des  Moines  consists  of  a  loud  flannel 
shirt,  heavy  white  athletic  socks,  and,  if  possible,  wavy  hair.  [32,  92] 

The  urgent  necessity  of  age-mate  contacts  for  these  teen-age  boys  is 
shown  in  the  following  comment: 

Many  of  Des  Moines's  teen-age  boys  have  jobs  after  school  but  all 
of  them  feel  that  life  would  be  unbearable  withput  at  least  four  hours 
of  "fooling  around"  every  day.  Fooling  around  consists  of  many 
things:  of  carrying  out  some  club  ritual,  of  playing  rummy  or  a  game 
of  catch  or  teasing  girls  or  holding  a  bull  session  under  the  awning 
in  front  of  the  drugstore.  [32,  96] 


256  RE-FORMATION  OF  THE  EGO  IN  ADOLESCENCE 

In  their  intensive  study  of  100  adolescent  boys  and  100  girls  in 
California,  Stolz,  M.  C.  Jones  and  Chaff ey  [41]  drew  definite  con- 
clusions regarding  the  influence  of  age-mate  reference  groups: 

As  we  look  back  over  this  three-year  period  during  which  we  have 
measured,  questioned,  watched  these  youngsters  in  the  early  stages  of 
adolescent  development,  certain  changes  in  interests,  attitudes,  and 
activities  seem  to  have  accompanied  the  physical  changes  and  to  be 
more  or  less  typical  of  the  group.  One  of  the  outstanding  facts  that 
we  have  noticed  about  these  children  as  they  grow  into  adolescence 
is  their  preoccupation  with  social  activities.  There  is  an  overwhelming 
desire  among  these  typical  junior  high  school  children  to  be  with  other 
children,  to  understand  themselves  in  their  relations  to  others  in  their 
age  group.  .  .  .  There  are  several  characteristics  of  this  phase  of  social 
awareness  which  distinguish  it  from  the  play  of  younger  children  and 
from  the  social  contacts  of  adults.  One  of  the  most  potent  drives 
behind  this  urge  for  social  activity  is  derived  from  the  youngsters' 
desire  for  group  approval.  To  achieve  this  approval  they  must  adapt 
themselves  to  the  ways  of  the  group,  substituting  its  standards  for 
those  of  the  home  and  the  school.  [41,  2] 

It  is  relevant  to  our  hypothesis  to  call  attention  to  the  fact  that 
these  boys  and  girls  are  the  same  adolescents  for  whom  adult 
approval  and  disapproval  meant  almost  nothing,  (p.  244)  The 
effect  of  age-mate  groups  to  which  the  adolescent  relates  himself 
may  be  so  strong  that  the  adolescent  may  even  feel  apologetic 
about  the  interference  of  his  parents.  One  of  the  girls  studied  in 
this  group  remarked: 

I  am  afraid  my  friends  will  think  I  have  no  control  over  my 
parents.  [41,  4] 

The  more  mature  group  influences  start  earlier  for  girls  than 
for  boys,  paralleling  the  conclusion  reported  earlier  in  this  chapter 
concerning  the  changing  body  and  changing  self.  The  girls 
almost  drag  the  boys  to  social  attitudes  and  interests  appropriate 
to  the  group.  Stolz,  Jones  and  Chaff  ey  [41]  state: 

A  point  to  be  noted  here  is  that  girls  begin  to  display  this  social 
awareness  and  interest  in  the  opposite  sex  earlier  than  boys.  Probably 
this  age  difference  would  be  greater  still  if  boys  were  allowed  to 
pursue  their  own  course  undisturbed.  But  actually  what  seems  to 


THE  EFFECTS  OF  AGE-MATE  REFERENCE  GROUPS  257 

happen  is  this:  the  girls,  feeling  the  urge  to  be  admired  by  boys,  start 
hunting  for  prospects  where  they  are  most  likely  to  be  found— in  the 
classroom  among  boys  of  their  own  age.  Then  begins  a  campaign 
by  the  girls  to  train  the  boys  as  good  dancing  partners  and  desirable 
party  escorts.  Under  this  tutelage,  sometimes  resented  by  the  boys, 
sometimes  preeningly  accepted,  boys  develop  the  required  social  atti- 
tudes about  a  year  later  than  the  girls,  many  of  them  earlier,  in  all 
probability,  than  if  they  were  left  to  their  own  devices.  [41,  5] 

The  dominance  of  group  interests  in  the  extracurricular  activities 
of  adolescents  has  been  observed  by  Cameron  [10]  who  reports 
that 

In  the  fall  of  the  second  year  no  reference  to  any  classroom  situation 
or  school  subject  appeared  in  our  records  of  conversation  or  in  the 
"scandal  sheets."  [10,  23] 

In  their  "self-imposed  isolation"  from  adults,  to  borrow  another 
characterization  from  Zachry,  appropriate  norms  may  arise  due  to 
the  group  interactions.  They  may  be  only  short-lived.  These 
norms  often  seem  to  serve  the  function  of  sanctioning  certain 
activities  otherwise  prohibited  by  society  at  large.  The  following 
observation  reported  by  Burks  [9]  gives  a  hint  in  this  direction: 

There  seems  to  be  a  hypothetical  danger  in  the  clubhouse  institution 
in  the  children's  lives — a  danger  that  was  only  suggested  by  some  of 
the  observations.  This  is  the  tendency  for  some  of  the  group  (espe- 
cially the  less  mature  youngsters)  to  promulgate  unacceptable  patterns 
of  behavior  that  started  in  the  clubhouse,  through  association  of  that 
behavior  with  the  surroundings  and  atmosphere  of  the  place  where 
the  patterns  developed.  Such  association  might  lead  to  a  sort  of 
"jelling"  of  anti-social  behavior,  which  would  be  further  fostered  by 
the  inertia  of  reputation  among  the  clubhouse  crowd.  [9,  27  /.] 

Among  the  adolescents  of  families  in  lower  income  groups,  in 
which  the  striving  for  some  sort  of  social  status  and  the  attempt 
to  overcome  economic  and  sex  deprivations  and  unfortunate 
family  backgrounds  are  all  combined  together,  group  conformi- 
ties may  end  with  more  grim  consequences.  In  Cressey's  study  of 
the  closed  dance  hall  in  Chicago  [13],  he  shows  how,  when  dep- 
rivations, conflict  with  grown-ups,  thwartings,  and  uncertainties 
become  strong  enough,  the  adolescent  girl  may  run  away  to  the 


258  RE-FORMATION  OF  THE  EGO  IN  ADOLESCENCE 

"closed  world"  of  the  dance  hall  and  anchor  herself  in  the  groups 
she  finds  there.  Most  of  these  girls  came  to  the  dance  hall  during 
adolescence.  They  came  from  broken  homes  and,  generally,  mis- 
erable conditions.  Usually,  there  was  severe  conflict  between  the 
girl  and  her  parents,  perhaps  because  the  latter's  Old  World  values 
were  widely  divergent  from  those  of  the  Americanized  adolescent 
One  girl  remarked: 

I  lived  with  other  dance  hall  girls,  met  my  fellows  at  the  dance  hall, 
got  my  living  from  the  dance  hall.  In  fact  there  was  nothing  I  wanted 
that  I  couldn't  get  through  it.  [13, 125] 

Another  said, 

I  don't  feel  like  I  belong  back  in  Wisconsin  any  more.  But  up  at 
the  "school"  I  just  feel  at  home.  ...  I  know  how  things  go,  I  have 
friends  who  are  always  glad  to  see  me  come  back,  and  who  really  are 
interested  enough  to  spend  their  money  on  me.  [13,  726] 

Cressey  concluded  that  the  "desire  for  recognition,  for  status,  along 
with  the  desire  for  intimacy  or  response,  and  for  new  experience 
and  excitement,  all  find  some  satisfactions  in  the  closed  dance 
hall."  [726]  But,  in  order  to  gain  these  satisfactions,  the  adolescent 
girl  must  become  one  of  the  group  of  dance  hall  girls.  For  ex- 
ample, one  girl  found  that  the  talk  in  the  rest  room  was  disgusting 
and  so  she  avoided  it.  As  a  result,  she  was  razzed  by  the  girls. 

But  it  didn't  take  long  to  get  used  to  things.  I  gradually  got  to 
using  their  talk  and  now  when  I  get  back  there  I  talk  dirty  just  like 
the  rest  of  them.  [13, 137] 

After  she  becomes  a  member  of  the  group,  it  is  not  difficult  for  the 
girl  to  turn  to  prostitution,  as  frequently  happens. 

The  approval  or  disapproval  of  the  reference  group  in  regulating 
the  attitudes  and  interests  of  adolescents  is  demonstrated  also  in 
the  case  of  youngsters  of  the  leisure  class.  Here  the  group  attitudes 
are  directed  towards  maintaining  social  distinction  and  class  lines. 
Adolescents  tend  to  anchor  themselves  in  groups  composed,  for 
the  most  part,  of  boys  and  girls  of  their  own  class.  Edith  Wharton 
[45]  described  such  a  group  in  the  1880's: 

Like  all  agreeable  societies,  ours  was  small,  and  the  people  com- 
posing it  met  almost  every  day,  and  always  sought  each  other  out  in 


THE  EFFECTS  OF  AGE-MATE  REFERENCE  GROUPS  259 

any  larger  company.  .  ,  .  Our  society  was,  in  short,  a  little  "set"  with 
its  private  catch-words,  observances  and  amusements,  and  its  indiffer- 
ence to  anything  outside  of  its  charmed  circle.  .  .  .  [45,  79,  italics 
ours] 

As  Ogden  [38]  has  pointed  out,  the  society  sets  today  have  more 
or  less  institutionalized  the  atmosphere  in  which  their  children  are 
brought  to  adolescence  through  the  private  boarding  school. 

The  homogeneous  character  of  the  student  body  is  conducive  to  the 
development  of  a  class  sense  of  cohesion.  [38,  34] 

Naturally,  this  limits  the  choice  of  friends. 

She  [the  society  girl]  is  cordial  without  discrimination  to  everyone 
who  is  accepted  in  her  sphere  of  society;  she  is  snobbish  to  outsiders. 
.  .  .  She  is  afraid  of  the  censure  of  her  crowd.  [38,  54,  italics  ours] 

When  a  society  girl  makes  her  debut,  at  eighteen,  she  may  become 
a  member  of  the  Junior  League,  which,  although  not  requiring  a 
debut  for  membership,  is  generally  composed  of  girls  who  have 
made  debuts.  The  Junior  League,  purportedly  organized  for  char- 
itable purposes,  seems  actually  to  serve  as  a  meeting  ground  of 
friends,  where  the  young  girl  carves  a  niche  for  herself  and  may 
stay  until  she  is  too  old  to  belong. 

Similarly,  college  fraternities  and  sororities  serve  as  groups  of 
"distinction."   As  one  writer  [6]  comments: 

The  word,  "sorority,"  connoting  culture,  refinement,  social  polish, 
has  become  synonymous  with  campus  prestige.  [6,  31] 

This  same  writer  tells  of  personal  experiences  in  a  well-known 
college  sorority  in  which  the  importance  of  maintaining  the  pres- 
tige of  the  sorority  was  so  great  that  it  was  worth  severe  financial 
strain.  An  "unorganized"  college  man  [24]  tells  how  the  attitudes 
generated  in  the  fraternity  reference  group  cut  across  group  an- 
tagonisms between  fraternities: 

I  soon  found  out  that  the  student  body  was  separated  by  a  wide 
and  formidable  gulf:  the  fraternity  members  as  a  whole  and  the 
independent  or  non-fraternity  group.  The  first  group  stalked  over 
the  campus  as  though  they  were  God's  chosen  children,  and  in  most 
cases  refused  to  have  any  association  with  the  despised  majority,  of 
which  I  was  one.  [24,  30] 


260  RE-FORMATION  OF  THE  EGO  IN  ADOLESCENCE 

Student  offices  were  ordinarily  held  by  fraternity  men,  elected  by 
the  amalgamated  efforts  of  the  fraternities.  And  when  an  "honor 
system"  with  no  supervision  for  examinations  was  proposed,  the 
"frat"  boys  united  to  push  it  through  so  that  "the  scholastic  stand- 
ing of  the  house  .  .  .  [would  be]  considered  honorable."  [30] 

We  must  not  ignore  individual  cases  of  strong  identification  seen 
in  intense  crushes,  love,  or  hero  worship.  In  such  instances,  ego- 
attitudes  are  shaped  and  oriented  in  many  different  ways  as  de- 
termined by  the  identification  with  the  lover,  sweetheart,  or  hero. 
Usually,  the  loved  persons  are  individuals  in  the  reference  group 
or  the  heroes  worshipped  are  persons  with  high  social  prestige,  in 
the  current  opinion  of  the  reference  group.  Even  in  such  cases  of 
intense  individual  identification,  the  norms  of  the  reference  group 
still  exert  a  significant  influence  on  the  ego-attitudes  of  the  indi- 
vidual. However,  if  the  idol  is  a  person  out  of  the  reference  group, 
then  the  individual  adolescent  becomes  a  social  misfit  in  his  or  her 
immediate  adolescent  surroundings.  Stolz,  M.  C.  Jones,  and 
Chaff ey  [41]  report  cases  of  high  school  girls  who  fell  in  love  with 
older  men  outside  of  their  own  adolescent  milieu  with  the  inten- 
tion of  marrying  them  and  who  became  socially  blind  to  their 
immediate  age-mate  surroundings.  [7]  Not  only  did  these  ado- 
lescent girls  satisfy  their  sex  urges  but  they  established  themselves 
psychologically  at  least  by  means  of  these  identifications. 

The  way  in  which  these  strong  identifications  with  persons 
(living  or  dead)  who  are  outside  the  current  prestige  scale  of  the 
immediate  age-mate  group  may  make  an  individual  a  social  misfit 
in  his  social  milieu  is  nicely  illustrated  by  a  case  described  by 
Hollingworth.  [20]  A  14-year-old  boy 

.  .  .  began  to  appear  in  his  classes  and  at  meals,  wearing  his  hat. 
He  steadfastly  refused  to  remove  his  headgear  at  the  request  of  parents 
and  teachers.  This  idiosyncrasy  caused  his  anxious  mother  several 
sleepless  nights.  Eventually  a  teacher  more  discerning  than  others 
obtained  the  lad's  confidential  account  of  his  peculiar  action.  He 
would  not  remove  his  hat,  because  William  Penn  (his  model  for  the 
moment)  had  refused  to  take  his  hat  off  in  assemblies!  [20, 179] 

But  even  such  exceptional  cases  do  not  vitiate  our  hypothesis.  For 
besides  any  sexual  or  other  functions  such  outside  heroes  may 


THE  EFFECTS  OF  AGE-MATE  REFERENCE  GROUPS  261 

serve,  they  do  serve  the  function  of  establishing  the  adolescent 
psychologically  and  at  least  for  the  time  being  as  a  person  in  his 
or  her  own  right. 

The  effect  of  age-mate  membership  groups  on  ego-attitudes.  In 
our  account  of  the  re-formation  of  the  ego  in  adolescence,  we  first 
put  the  problem  in  its  broad  social  setting.  This  gave  us  the  per- 
spective necessary  to  realize  the  impact  of  special  socioeconomic 
variations  in  producing  variations  in  ego  problems,  making  the 
process  more  difficult  in  some  societies  than  in  others,  prolonging 
it  more  in  certain  societies  than  in  others.  We  saw  how  the  diffi- 
culties and  complications  involved  in  the  transition  from  childhood 
to  adulthood  led  to  adult-youth  conflict.  This,  in  turn,  led  to 
intensified  efforts  on  the  part  of  the  adolescents  to  achieve  inde- 
pendence from  adults  and  to  establish  themselves  as  persons  in 
their  own  right.  We  saw  how  adolescents  gravitate  towards  each 
other,  forming  age-mate  groups  that  become  of  supreme  impor- 
tance in  their  lives  at  least  during  this  transition  period.  Whatever 
status  value  the  adolescent  genuinely  experiences  during  this  time 
of  transition  is  derived  from  his  standing  in  this  reference  group 
of  age  mates. 

The  age-mate  reference  group,  however,  defines  identifications 
and  personal  preferences  only  in  broad  outlines  and  only  for 
standards  and  fads  common  to  all  who  relate  themselves  to  these 
groups.  In  the  "self-imposed"  psychological  isolation  of  the  ado- 
lescent, further  differentiation  spontaneously  takes  place.  Strong 
friendships  or  cliques  of  two,  three,  or  more  adolescents  emerge 
on  the  basis  of  common  secrets  (sexual  and  otherwise),  common 
desires,  common  problems,  common  interests  such  as  those  based 
on  family  background,  school  activities,  and  the  like.  In  such 
group  formations,  the  members  share  secrets  which  they  cannot 
and  dare  not  share  with  others,  especially  grown-ups.  Certain 
roles  arise  prescribing  the  relative  standing  of  members  to  each 
other.  Certain  expectations  are  produced  which  prescribe  mutual 
loyalties  and  responsibilities.  Certain  badges  of  belongingness, 
certain  catchwords,  and  even  norms  (though  perhaps  short  lived) 
arise,  leading  to  attitudes  appropriate  for  group  members. 

Such  cliques,  as  Jones,  Zachry,  and  others  have  pointed  out,  are 
formed  first  among  girls  gnd  then  among  boys.  (Another  fact 


262  RE-FORMATION  OF  THE  EGO  IN  ADOLESCENCE 

that  demonstrates  the  earlier  social  maturity  of  girls  following 
their  earlier  physical  maturation.)  The  formation  of  cliques  or 
gangs  made  up  of  both  sexes  may  follow.  In  some  cases,  the 
clique  formations  of  two  or  three  close  friends  may  serve  as  nuclei 
around  which  large  gangs  are  formed.  Such  cliques  or  gangs 
may  survive  for  a  shorter  or  longer  period  owing  to  the  more  or 
less  "mercurial  nature"  of  adolescents'  relationships.  These  cliques 
or  gangs  may  be  more  or  less  confidential  or  secret  formations, 
depending  on  the  character  of  the  activities  in  which  they  engage 
and  the  degree  of  conflict  with  adults  and  various  institutions. 
The  clique  or  gang  gains  solidarity  in  proportion  to  the  resistances 
it  meets  and  withstands.  So  long  as  they  last,  these  cliques  or 
gangs  demand  conformity  and  loyalty  from  their  members.  In- 
dividuals who  do  not  conform  are  ostracized  or  discriminated 
against.  The  status  maintained  in  the  clique  or  gang  is  the  main 
source  of  ego-satisfaction.  The  sense  of  group  belongingness  in 
the  clique  or  gang  brings  a  feeling  of  personal  stability  or  security 
in  proportion  to  the  degree  of  psychological  weaning  from  parents 
or  the  intensity  of  parent-youth  conflict. 

Many  authors  in  the  field  of  adolescence  have  given  accounts  of 
the  importance  of  such  clique  or  gang  formations  or  have  called 
attention  to  the  way  in  which  they  determine  the  character  of 
adolescent  attitudes  and  identifications.  Out  of  a  host  of  such 
reports  we  shall  choose  only  a  few  representative  examples. 
Goodenough  [17]  points  out  the  significance  of  the  solidarity  and 
group  consciousness  that  prevail  among  gang  members: 

Not  only  does  the  adolescent,  as  a  rule,  begin  to  show  a  new  interest 
in  the  opposite  sex,  but  a  new  element  appears  in  his  relationships 
with  persons  of  his  own  sex.  This  is  the  formation  of  clubs  or  gangs. 
It  is,  of  course,  true  that  long  before  the  age  of  adolescence  children 
play  together  in  groups  and  form  special  friendships  that  give  these 
groups  something  of  a  lasting  character.  But  in  'most  cases  the  social 
groups  formed  by  young  children  lac\  the  solidarity  and  the  feeling 
of  group-consciousness  that  characterize  the  adolescent  gang  or  club. 
[17,  491,  italics  ours] 

Wile  [47]  generalizes  as  follows  concerning  the  importance  of 
adolescent  groups: 


THE  EFFECTS  OF  AGE-MATE  REFERENCE  GROUPS  263 

Much  of  adolescent  adjustment  depends  upon  the  relative  degree  of 
self-consciousness  and  group  consciousness.  It  also  rests  upon  the 
acceptance  of  oneself  as  part  of  the  group  and  the  willingness  to 
identify  the  demands  of  the  group  as  in  harmony  with  one's  own 
needs.  Herein  appears  the  potential  significance  of  group  pressures, 
seeking  to  bring  about  adolescent  conformity  with  the  mores  of  the 
age.  Every  group  that  invites  adolescents  to  unite  with  it  bids  them 
live  in  accord  with  group  sanctions — and  each  group  is  concerned  with 
its  own  goals  and  principles  which  may  not  be  in  the  interest  of 
society  as  a  whole.  Every  social  group  molds  adolescents  in  direct 
ratio  to  their  participation.  Adolescent  groups  naturally  are  most 
directive  and  may  be  coercive.  The  gang  and  the  club,  as  sub-sections 
of  society,  exert  their  influence  for  or  against  the  ruling  principles  of 
the  world  of  grown-ups.  The  shifting  morals  of  each  age  help  to 
determine  the  extent  of  the  fight  of  society  for,  and  of  youth  against, 
specific  indoctrinations.  Concepts  of  right  and  wrong,  good  and  bad, 
moral  and  immoral,  decent  and  indecent,  constitute  a  risk  for  all 
acceptances  extending  beyond  the  simple  ideas  that  continually  find 
favor  among  adolescents  upon  the  basis  of  reasonable  tradition.  [47, 
4f.,  italics  ours] 

Zachry  [49]  points  to  some  of  the  factors  which  make  gang 
standards  become  the  adolescent's  own  standards: 

It  is  significant  also  that  in  his  compliance  with  gang  standards  he 
is  taking  a  step  toward  self-determination  in  conduct,  since  this  group 
is  made  up  not  of  those  who  are  much  larger  and  stronger  than  he 
but  of  those  who  are  like  him  in  appearance,  capacities,  and  interests. 
Thus  these  standards  are  more  nearly  his  own  than  were  those  which 
he  acquired  so  early  that  he  does  not  remember  how  this  came  about, 
and  some  of  which  he  is  now  relinquishing  for  the  time  being  at  least. 
[49, 163  f.] 

The  formation  of  adolescent  gangs  and  the  function  they  serve 
are  clearly  stated  by  Peter  Bios  [5] : 

In  early  adolescence  the  formation  of  gangs  represents  the  begin- 
ning of  a  group  life  which  has  its  own  distinctive  characteristics. 
[5,249] 

A  few  pages  later,  Bios  writes: 

It  offers  him  in  return  a  security  in  group  belongingness  and  in 
collective  responsibility  at  a  time  when  he  is  abandoning  childhood 


264  RE-FORMATION  OP  THE  EGO  IN  ADOLESCENCE 

relationships  and  reorienting  himself  in  terms  of  mature  goals.  In 
response  to  the  pressures  of  peer  culture,  his  family  patterns  of  rela- 
tionship, identification,  and  feeling  life  are  gradually  modified  in  the 
direction  of  group  norms.  [5,  254,  italics  ours] 

Commenting  on  the  resistance  these  norms  and  their  conforming 
behavior  encounter  from  grown-ups,  Bios  points  out  the  difficult 
state  of  marginality  the  adolescent  experiences  in  his  efforts  to 
establish  himself: 

Such  adult  indifference  or  hostility  towards  the  adolescent's  treas- 
ured peer  standards  raises  a  further  difficulty:  if  he  is  loyal  to  his 
group,  he  denies  himself  adult  acceptance  and  approval;  if  he  com- 
plies with  adult  demands,  evading  the  possibility  of  asserting  his 
independence,  he  loses  the  recognition  of  his  peers.  [5,  254] 

We  shall  be  concerned  again  in  the  next  chapter  with  the  un- 
pleasant consequences  of  marginality  or  conflicting  identifications. 

These  representative  statements  are  not  mere  armchair  specula- 
tions of  authorities.  As  we  shall  see  from  the  illustrations  in  this 
and  the  next  chapter,  they  are  amply  substantiated  by  factual 
evidence. 

The  systematic  observations  of  the  social  activities  and  groupings 
in  a  high  school  clubhouse  during  the  years  1936-37  unmistakably 
show  clique  and  gang  formations.  We  should  like  to  call  atten- 
tion to  the  fact  that  the  situation  in  the  clubhouse  was  not  alto- 
gether free  from  adult  supervision.  F.  W,  Burks  [9],  who  sum- 
marized the  clubhouse  observations,  expresses  the  formation  of 
adolescent  cliques  or  gangs  in  terms  of  "social  stratification." 9 

One  of  the  most  notable  manifestations  of  the  adolescent  group, 
which  began  during  this  year,  and  continued  during  the  following 
years  after  the  clubhouse  was  closed,  was  the  differentiation  into  dis- 
tinct social  strata,  between  which  there  was  little  real  intercourse  and 
each  of  which  built  up  its  own  barriers,  defenses,  and  feelings  of 
solidarity.  [9, 16] 

When  once  formed,  these  groups,  which  generated  "a  growing 
feeling  of  cliquishness,"  demanded  that  their  members  behave 

9  We  should  prefer  to  save  the  term  "social  stratification"  to  designate  major 
class  delineations  in  society  at  large  in  terms  of  the  individual's  role  in  the  processes 
of  production,  distribution,  and  the  like:  for  example,  as  employer  or  employed. 


THE  EFFECTS  OF  AGE-MATE  REFERENCE  GROUPS  265 

within  the  bounds  of  the  group  standards  and  keep  in  step  with 
them.  Those  who  went  beyond  these  bounds  were  ostracized  or 
discriminated  against. 

The  approaches  made  by  "novices"  to  be  accepted  by  those  who 
have  already  established  themselves  were  observed. 

In  addition  to  the  rising  stratification  on  the  basis  of  prestige  in  the 
social  hierarchy  of  the  school,  there  were  also  other  bases  for  dis- 
crimination and  feelings  of  superiority.  One  of  the  most  striking  was 
degree  of  social  maturity,  the  less  mature  boys  feeling  at  a  disadvan- 
tage with  more  experienced  ones,  hesitating  to  assert  themselves,  or 
even  to  stand  up  for  their  rights.  The  less  mature  were  subject  to 
disapproval  for  some  of  their  activities  by  the  "older"  group;  and 
frequently  compensated  for  their  feelings  of  inferiority  by  swaggering 
attempts  to  appear  grown-up.  [9, 19] 

On  the  basis  of  such  observations,  Harold  Jones  [25]  points  out 
the  difficulties  experienced  by  non-clique  members  or  leftovers: 

The  social  stratification  encountered  in  high  school  affected  the  girls 
much  more  than  the  boys.  Yet  it  was  the  boys  more  than  the  girls 
from  Jackson  Junior  High  who  were  outspoken  about  the  treatment 
which  they  received  at  the  hands  of  the  cliques.  Although  certain 
positions  (such  as  student  body  president)  were  available  to  students 
who  were  not  members  of  these  exclusive  groups,  other  offices  dealing 
with  the  school  social  functions  were  traditionally  filled  by  a  "club" 
member.  [25,  22] 

Jones  gives  a  concise  and  concrete  picture  of  such  clique  for- 
mations in  the  form  of  Moreno  sociograms  obtained  from  actual 
data.  He  titles  this  section  "Group  Structures."  Here  we  repro- 
duce sociograms  showing  the  interpersonal  relationships  of  boys 
and  girls  approximately  15  years  old.10  It  can  be  clearly  seen  from 
Figure  1  that  among  the  girls  in  the  high  ninth  grade,  "the  social 
structure,  at  this  age  level,  consisted  of  a  number  of  compact 
and  somewhat  separated  groups  or  cliques."  [43]  This  compact 

10  These  sociograms  are  reproduced  from  Campbell's  study  [11]  with  the 
permission  of  the  author.  They  are  also  reproduced  by  Jones  [25,  44].  The 
sociograms  were  constructed  from  the  results  of  an  item  on  the  "Guess  Who"  test 
asking  for  the  subject's  "best  friends.'*  The  dark  lines  represent  reciprocal  men- 
tions of  "best  friends,"  while  die  broken  lines  represent  mentions  not  reciprocated. 


266 


RE-FORMATION  OF  THE  EGO  IN  ADOLESCENCE 


cliquishness  is  not  evident  in  the  sociogram  for  boys  at  this  age 
level  (Figure  2).  This  fact  shows  once  more  the  earlier  social 
maturity  of  girls.  It  would  be  interesting  to  trace  the  interpersonal 
relationships  of  boys  a  year  or  so  older.  We  should  guess  that 
clique  formations  among  the  boys  would  also  increase  when  they 
reached  a  corresponding  level  of  social  maturity. 


8a 


Nonreciprocal  mention 
Reciprocal  mention 


FIG.  1.    Best  friends — girls. 


Analysis  of  the  consequences  of  being  "left  over,"  or  of  having 
no  established  standing  in  the  reference  group  and  no  member- 
ship in  a  closely  knit  clique,  shows  in  clear-cut  fashion  the  psy- 
chological properties  that  arise  in  group  membership  or  from 
group  identification.  This  is  revealed  in  the  case  of  John  Sanders, 
studied  by  Jones  and  his  associates.  John  is  the  same  boy  referred 
to  previously  in  our  discussion  of  the  changing  body  and  chang- 
ing self.  In  comparison  to  his  age  mates,  he  was  retarded  a  year 
or  so  in  his  physical  development.  We  have  already  noted  the 


THE  EFFECTS  OF  AGE-MATE  REFERENCE  GROUPS 


267 


important  psychological  consequences  of  such  retardment.  In 
Figure  2  John  is  represented  by  circle  78  in  the  upper  right  corner 
of  the  sociogram.  As  can  be  readily  seen,  he  was  in  a  peripheral 
social  situation.  He  felt  the  unpleasant  experience,  the  insecurity 


K^-^?^si 

^«~  /    \  /  >&• 


v    x^C^*»>  '   /*"\  \  /  s  *'     i\?wj 

'Wi 


^—-77 

**P\  f  / 


~*~  Nonreciprocal  mention 
—  Reciprocal  mention 

FIG.  2.    Best  friends — boys. 

of  his  peripheral  state  in  relation  to  his  immediate  reference  group. 
At  this  time,  he  was  at  the  high  point  of  unpopularity  among  his 
age  mates,  in  spite  of  the  fact  that  he  made  "unsuccessful  attempts 
to  gain  attention  and  to  identify  himself  with  his  classmates."  [57] 

John  is  obviously  the  kind  of  person  with  whom  no  girl  would  go 
out,  if  she  thought  she  had  prestige  to  maintain.  Even  girls  who  are 
fairly  independent  in  their  attitudes  toward  their  social  position  would 
feel  that  they  couldn't  descend  to  go  out  with  John  Sanders!  Perhaps 
the  chief  reason  for  this  is  his  lack  of  a  functional  familiarity  with 
social  patterns;  he  tries  hard  to  conform  to  these  standards  of  the 
adolescent  culture,  but  is  still  an  outsider  wistfully  looking  in.  [25,  57] 


268  RE-FORMATION  OF  THE  EGO  IN  ADOLESCENCE 

This  statement  was  verified  by  Tryon's  research  [44]  on  the 
popularity  of  boys  and  girls  15  years  old  in  this  same  social  milieu, 
perhaps  these  very  boys  and  girls: 

Boys,  to  be  successful  with  girls,  must  be  admired  by  boys.  .  .  . 
[44,565] 

John's  "unskillfulness  in  activities  enjoyed  by  other  boys  marked 
him  as  'queer';  it  made  for  social  isolation,  and  his  response  to 
this  lowered  status  was  to  develop  characteristics  which  further 
increased  his  unpopularity."  [159f.]  This  social  isolation  from 
the  group  was  deeply  resented  by  John  himself.  As  he  expressed 
it  to  his  counselor  in  his  senior  year  in  high  school : 

The  greatest  mistake  I  ever  made  was  not  spending  more  time  on 
the  playgrounds.  I  should  have  been  made  to  do  it.  Boys  don't  like 
you  unless  you  can  play  games.  It  affects  your  whole  personality. 
[25,  103] 

John  was  interested  in  art,  perhaps  as  a  compensation  for  his 
lack  of  athletic  prowess  and  other  activities  popular  among  the 
boys  of  his  reference  group.  And  he  was  pretty  good  at  it.  But 
among  his  age  mates  at  the  early  years  of  adolescence,  artistic 
accomplishment  was  not  a  criterion  used  in  judging  acceptability. 
As  we  saw  before,  after  John's  delayed  "adolescent  spurt"  took 
place,  his  popularity  also  jumped  up.  The  consequences  for  John's 
popularity  of  this  "adolescent  spurt"  and  the  changing  interests 
of  the  group  in  later  adolescent  years  are  concisely  formulated  by 
Jones: 

In  later  adolescence  these  deficiencies  became  of  smaller  importance, 
partly  because  of  changing  social  values  and  partly  also  because  of  a 
process  of  maturing  that  is  expressed  in  social  structures  as  well  as  in 
organisms.  The  naive  likemindedness  characteristic  of  a  younger 
group  broadens,  at  a  later  age,  into  a  greater  variety  of  special  inter- 
ests. Special  sympathies  and  appreciations  in  this  more  differentiated 
society  give  the  individual  a  greater  range  of  choice — a  greater  oppor- 
tunity to  select  his  own  appropriate  environment.  .  .  .  Thus,  the  more 
favorable  situation  and  prognosis  for  John,  as  he  reached  the  end  of 
senior  high  school,  were  due  partly  to  changes  in  John  as  he  labori- 
ously caught  up  with  the  group,  but  we  must  give  credit  also  to 


THE  EFFECTS  OF  AGE-MATE  REFERENCE  GROUPS  269 

changes  in  the  group  as  they  caught  up  with  John,  and  as  their  values 
and  standards  of  achievement  came  closer  to  the  sober  aspirations 
which  John  had  always  held  important.  [25, 160  f.] 

In  Burks'  summary  [9]  of  the  Tugwell  clubhouse,  more  evi- 
dence of  changing  values  within  the  adolescent  reference  group  is 
given.  For  example,  as  the  group  became  older,  the  clubhouse  no 
longer  served  as  the  center  of  activities.  As  standards  of  amuse- 
ment become  more  sophisticated,  such  as  attendance  at  night  clubs 
and  country  clubs,  the  clubhouse  was  occupied  chiefly  by  "young- 
sters who  had  not  been  able,  because  of  social  immaturity  or  lack 
of  family  'status'  to  achieve  a  place  in  the  various  social  groups  in 
the  high  school.  .  .  ."  [28]  Just  as  an  individual  (for  example, 
John  Sanders)  may  be  a  misfit  in  his  reference  group,  so  too 
certain  cliques  may  be  generally  looked  down  on  as  "queer."  In 
the  clubhouse,  the  art  group  "had  no  prestige  with  the  general 
crowd,"  because  its  activities  did  not  fit  into  the  dominant  values 
current  among  the  larger  adolescent  group.  Whatever  the  diffi- 
culties involved  in  belonging  to  a  low  prestige  group,  still,  as 
Burks  remarks,  at  least  a  member  would  avoid  the  "thwarted 
feeling"  of  having  no  group  at  all. 

Group  formations,  such  as  those  found  in  the  California  studies, 
are  not  artifacts  of  biased  observation.  Such  groups  and  cliques 
can  be  found  in  concrete  observations  of  everyday  life.  Life  maga- 
zine [30]  had  one  of  its  reporters  make  a  picture  survey  of  an 
adolescent  clique  composed  of  12  girls  from  15  to  17  years  old. 
The  survey  reveals  that  all  of  these  girls  "go  to  the  same  school, 
take  the  same  courses,  know  the  same  people  and  generally  ex- 
hibit the  passionate  uniformity  of  a  teen-age  clique."  [92  f.]  These 
girls  spent  most  of  their  time  together,  exchanged  confidences, 
spent  long  hours  on  the  telephone.  Group  solidarity  and  loyalty 
"keeps  the  girls  firmly  united  against  all  protest"  from  grown-ups 
concerning  their  activities  or  current  fads. 

Another  Life  survey  [31],  made  in  Indianapolis,  revealed  that 
the  currently  prevalent  "Sub-Deb  Clubs"  serve  the  same  psycho- 
logical function  as  does  any  spontaneously  formed  adolescent 
clique.  Any  group  of  girls  may  form  a  Sub-Deb  Club  which  is 
free  from  adult  supervision.  At  the  time  of  the  survey  (1945) 
there  were  in  Indianapolis  700  such  clubs  with  some  6,000  members. 


270  RE-FORMATION  OF  THE  EGO  IN  ADOLESCENCE 

Sub-Deb  Clubs  have  meetings  and  initiations  and  even  print  news- 
papers, but  their  main  purpose  is  to  have  parties.  In  Indianapolis  they 
are  enormously  successful  in  achieving  this  purpose.  One  reason  is 
that  single  girls  might  be  shy  about  asking  boys  to  parties  but  clubs 
of  girls  can  be  downright  aggressive.  [31,  91] 

This  comment  of  Life's  reporter  is  similar  to  those  made  by  Stolz, 
M.  C.  Jones,  and  Chaff ey  [41]  in  California,  indicating  that  the 
girls,  earlier  in  maturing,  tend  to  "drag"  the  boys  into  social  activi- 
ties. These  cliques,  like  many  gangs,  have  names  and  even  pins 
which  serve  to  strengthen  the  group  and  give  an  identification  to 
in-group  members.  Some  of  these  names  are  clues  to  the  changing 
interests  of  these  adolescent  girls.  For  example,  Swami  stands  for 
"Subtle  Women  are  Most  Intriguing";  Jilts  means  "Jump  in  Line 
to  Smooch";  Genius,  Inc.  refers  to  the  talents  of  its  drama-loving 
members;  while  the  meaning  of  Glama  is  the  secret  of  the  mem- 
bers alone. 

Although  these  Sub-Deb  Clubs  are  not  exclusively  secret  socie- 
ties, the  members  do  share  confidences.  Goodenough  [17]  writes: 

The  girl's  club  is  less  likely  to  center  around  any  particular  activity. 
Its  members,  however,  share  among  themselves  various  "secrets,"  bits 
of  gossip,  and  so  on  to  which  they  often  refer  mysteriously  in  the 
presence  of  outsiders  in  the  hope  of  arousing  curiosity  and  envy.  [17, 
492] 

It  has  frequently  been  noted  that  in  the  United  States  there  is 
relatively  greater  secrecy  in  the  clique  activities  among  girls  than 
among  boys,  due  perhaps  to  the  more  conflicting  ego-attitudes  of 
femininity. 

Campbell's  research  [11]  has  already  been  mentioned  in  con- 
nection with  Jones's  discussion  of  group  structures.  Although 
Campbell  specifically  studied  adolescent  opinions  of  classmates  of 
their  own  and  the  opposite  sex,  her  results  also  have  implications 
concerning  the  problem  of  clique  formation  among  boys  and  girls. 
Reference  to  Figures  1  and  2  taken  from  Campbell's  study  show 
that  "cliques  of  two,  three  or  more  individuals  seem  to  be  fairly 
descriptive  of  the  girls,"  while  the  boys  of  this  age  tend  to  associ- 
ate in  "chains"  of  friendship,  not  in  cliques.  We  would  be  in- 
clined to  agree  with  Campbell's  conclusion  that  most  of  these  sex 


THE  EFFECTS  OF  AGE-MATE  REFERENCE  GROUPS  271 

differences  are  related  to  "the  greater  physiological  and  social 
maturity  of  the  girls/'  and  that  consideration  of  boys  of  an  equal 
level  of  maturity  would  reveal  more  similar  results.  However,  in 
line  with  our  hypothesis  that  the  degree  of  absorption  in  and  the 
degree  of  solidarity  of  age-mate  groupings  varies  with  the  degree 
of  conflict,  some  questions  posed  by  Campbell  may  be  considered. 

Do  women  in  our  culture  feel  less  security  of  independent  status  so 
that  there  is  greater  need  of  the  ego  support  derived  from  identification 
with  a  group?  What  is  the  effect  on  personality?  Are  women  more 
conservative  in  their  ideas,  more  responsive  to  personal  influence  and 
loyalties?  .  .  .  Does  this  type  of  organization  tend  toward  a  stereo- 
typing of  personality,  interests,  attitudes,  opinions?  [11,  150] 

In  view  of  material  on  gang  formation  presented  in  the  next 
chapter,  we  can  safely  say  that  the  greater  cliquish  tendency  found 
among  15-year-old  girls  in  California  in  this  study  is  not  always 
in  all  strata  of  society,  under  all  conditions,  greater  than  for  boys. 
Additional  research  on  older  adolescents  comparable  to  the  upper 
and  middle  class  children  in  this  study  would  give  valuable  clues 
relevant  to  the  preceding  hypothesis  concerning  the  degree  of 
absorption  in,  the  solidarity,  and  the  secrecy  of  adolescent  groups. 
Like  any  other  group  at  any  age  level,  an  adolescent  group,  once 
formed  and  stabilized,  demands  conformity  and  loyalty  (identifi- 
cation) from  individual  members.  The  status  members  really 
experience  is  derived  largely  from  their  standing  in  the  clique  or 
gang  and  is  proportional  to  the  degree  of  their  identification  with 
the  clique  or  gang.  Behavior  that  is  for  them  proper  and  im- 
proper, decent  and  indecent  is  regulated  by  the  current  group 
norms,  as  long  as  they  last  and  as  long  as  the  individual  enjoys 
group  membership.  From  an  unpublished  California  study,11  we 
cite  a  concrete  case  showing  the  observance  of  an  adolescent  girl 
of  rigid  norms  governing  the  acceptance  of  dates: 

With  respect  to  the  group  codes  connected  with  "going  steady"  she 
seems  to  have  accepted  all  the  necessary  accompaniments— "I  can't  go 
out  with  Fred  on  Friday."  (Fred  was  coming  to  see  her  for  a  date 
she  had  had  for  some  time.)  "So  I  just  told  him  that  I  couldn't  go 

11  Unpublished  material  used  through  the  kindness  of  Harold  and  Mary  Cover 
Jones. 


272  RE-FORMATION  OF  THE  EGO  IN  ADOLESCENCE 

out  with  him  and  if  he  comes  it's  just  too  bad!"  When  I  suggested 
tentatively  the  possibility  of  breaking  the  date  with  her  new  "steady" 
she  responded  with  immediate  certainty  that  that  just  couldn't  be 
done  "because  I'm  going  steady  now  and  I  just  can't  go  out  with 
anybody  else." 

Although  activity  can  proceed  at  a  rapid  pace,  limits  to  behavior 
are  prescribed  by  group  norms.  If  anybody  goes  beyond  these 
bounds  he  is  put  within  the  bounds  again.  This  is  keenly  observed 
by  Cameron  [10]  in  his  study  of  200  junior  high  school  pupils: 

To  have  news  value,  the  competition  of  boys  and  girls  for  popularity 
with  members  of  the  opposite  sex,  must  be  expressed  in  exaggerated, 
daring,  and  exciting  terms.  But  their  verbal  freedom  is  in  general 
divorced  by  a  wide  margin  from  their  actions;  so  that,  for  example, 
anyone  who  became  really  interested  in  heavy  petting  was  ostracized 
from  this  group.  [10,  20] 

Similarly,  the  reporter  who  made  the  picture  survey  of  a  high 
school  clique  for  Life  [30]  includes  pictures  showing  that  a  mem- 
ber of  the  clique  runs  the  danger  of  ostracism  if  she  is  too  inter- 
ested in  boys,  puts  herself  in  their  midst  ignoring  the  girls,  "necks" 
at  movies,  or  has  too  many  dates.  Cameron  observes  further: 

Let  anyone  get  conceited  about  his  status,  or  ride  too  high  on  a 
wave  of  popularity,  and  without  warning  a  torrent  of  invectives  will 
be  let  loose.  With  merciless  directness  and  intolerance  the  offending 
person's  prestige  is  battered  into  shreds  and  he  is  left  to  fend  for  him- 
self in  getting  back  into  the  group.  [10, 22] 

In  the  next  chapter  we  shall  elaborate  our  account  of  the  struc- 
tural properties  arising  in  spontaneously  formed  groups  or  gangs. 
We  shall  give  there  concrete  illustrations  of  the  impact  of  these 
structural  properties  of  groups  on  the  identifications,  loyalties,  and 
attitudes  of  the  individual  members.  Investigators,  both  in  psy- 
chology and  sociology,  who  studied  the  behavior  of  the  individual 
member  in  adolescent  gangs  or  groups,  have  furnished  invaluable 
data  which  are  ripe  for  a  social  psychological  conceptualization. 
We  include  here  only  a  few  more  examples  to  show  the  serious 
individual  consequences  that  can  result  from  allegiance  to  group 
norms. 


THE  EFFECTS  OF  AGE-MATE  REFERENCE  GROUPS  273 

The  concrete  cases  studied  by  Thomas  [42]  highlight  the  serious 
effects  such  group  memberships  can  have  on  the  behavior  of  s'ome 
unfortunate  youngsters. 

It  frequently  happens  also  that  a  girl  is  drawn  or  drifts  out  of  her 
family  and  community  into  a  bad  gang,  as  in  the  case  No.  78,  becomes 
identified  with  them  by  assimilation,  and  cannot  free  herself.  She 
may  then  be  kept  by  one  of  the  men  or  sold  into  a  house.  Cases  No. 
79  and  No.  80  are  typical  of  the  psychology  of  the  girl  in  this  relation. 
[42, 142] 

Augusta  Jameson  [23]  who  actually  lived  with  a  group  of  delin- 
quent girls  without  identifying  herself  with  the  authorities  until 
she  secured  the  girls'  confidence,  secured  29  autobiographies  and 
questionnaires  from  106  delinquent  girls.  These  questionnaires 
were  made  through  the  aid  of  some  of  the  girls  themselves,  and 
were  given  twice,  four  months  apart.  Correlations  between  the 
test  and  retest  were  "very  high."  Basing  her  findings  on  this 
material  plus  "agency,  institutional,  and  court  records;  and  some 
upon  physical,  psychiatric,  and  psychological  examinations,"  Jame- 
son concludes: 

In  general  there  appears  to  be  a  vague  pattern,  an  outline  of  succes- 
sive incidents,  and  conditions,  in  these  girls'  lives.  These  various  stages 
do  not  necessarily  occur  singly  but  often  in  combination  with  previous 
or  subsequent  ones.  [23, 30] 

Among  these  conditions,  Jameson  lists  the  following  which  indi- 
cate the  effects  of  age-mate  groups: 

Failure  of  the  family  and  the  community  to  offer  normal  socially 
accepted  satisfactions  for  their  personal  needs.  .  .  .  Existence  in  the 
community  of  an  apparently  congenial  group  of  young  people  par- 
ticipating in  overt  sex  activity.  .  .  .  Initiatory  experiences  of  the  girl 
in  the  sex  group,  frequently  accompanied  by  fear  and  unpleasantness, 
but  necessary  as  a  "price  of  admission,"  to  this  group  whose  members 
appear  to  be  having  adequate  satisfactions.  .  .  .  Adoption  by  the  girl 
of  the  pattern  of  sex  delinquency  defined  by  the  group.  [23,  31] 

Jameson  then  concludes: 

It  is  the  writer's  impression  that  the  delinquency  of  these  girls  has 
been  a  normal,  natural  process,  resulting  from  the  type  of  social  stimuli 


274  RE-FORMATION  OF  THE  EGO  IN  ADOLESCENCE 

to  which  they  have  been  exposed.  From  the  girls'  standpoint,  their 
delinquency  has  represented  an  opportunity  for  securing  the  human 
satisfactions  which  in  more  conventional  social  situations  are  secured 
in  more  socially  approved  forms  of  conduct  [23, 31] 

Such  group  determinations  of  more  or  less  consequential  types  of 
behavior  are  amply  illustrated  in  the  next  chapter. 

SOME  IMPLICATIONS  OF  THE  FOREGOING  MATERIAL  FOR  OUR 

PROBLEM 

Before  closing  our  sketchy  account  of  the  psychology  of  the 
re-formation  of  the  ego  during  adolescence,  a  concise  statement  of 
the  main  trend  that  stands  out,  especially  in  the  last  sections,  will 
provide  a  useful  clarification.  This  trend  has  serious  implications 
that  have  to  be  accounted  for  in  any  psychology  of  what  is  usually 
designated  in  a  loose  way  as  "ego  drives" — an  important  domain 
in  the  field  of  motivation.  As  was  hinted  before,  the  appallingly 
long  and  disconnected  lists  of  adolescent  attitudes  and  interests 
will  gain  a  coherent  scheme  of  conceptualization  if  they  are  studied 
first  in  relation  to  the  individual  boy's  or  girl's  reference  groups  in 
general  and  membership  groups  in  particular.  For  the  adolescent 
derives  his  major  attitudes  and  interests  from  the  reference  groups 
to  which  he  relates  himself.  As  we  have  seen  before,  the  norms  of 
these  reference  groups  become  his  attitudes.  The  major  status 
problems  for  him  arise  in  relation  to  his  reference  groups  in  general 
and  to  his  membership  groups  in  particular.  His  identifications, 
loyalties,  and  conformities  are,  in  a  major  way,  determined  in 
relation  to  them.  His  main  concern,  for  the  time  being  at  least, 
is  to  establish  himself,  to  have  a  standing  in  his  reference  and 
membership  groups,  even  though  they  may  change  in  different 
periods.  The  consequences  of  this  concern  define  in  an  important 
way  many  aspects  of  his  ego  problems. 

Of  course,  any  standing  in  the  reference  group  or  membership 
group  will  not  do.  Individual  members  strive  to  achieve  better 
standing  in  their  group  due  to  the  factors  of  individual  differences 
in  temperament,  intelligence,  ability,  and  in  many  other  dimen- 
sions. These  individual  differences  may  lead  to  varieties  of  con- 


SOME  IMPLICATIONS  FOR  OUR  PROBLEM  275 

tinuous  efforts  to  better  relative  standing  in  the  group,  may  cause 
frictions  among  the  members,  may  end  in  discriminations,  ostra- 
cisms, and  such.  However,  all  of  these  effects  of  individual  differ- 
ences take  place  within  the  terms  of  group  loyalty  and  conformity 
prescribed  by  the  group  norms,  whatever  their  character  and 
direction  may  be  in  the  particular  social  and  class  setting  in  which 
the  individual  moves.  If  he  displays  behavior  altogether  incom- 
patible with  the  bounds  of  group  characteristics,  he  is  simply  out; 
he  becomes  an  outsider.  In  some  cases,  any  status  in  the  group  is 
of  more  value  to  the  individual  than  loss  of  the  sense  of  belonging- 
ness,  especially  if  he  is  in  conflict  with  unfortunate  family  circum- 
stances, (p.  298)  All  of  these  conclusions  indicate  that  the  all 
important  factors  of  individual  differences  have  meaning  in  group 
situations  as  they  relate  to  the  relative  standing  of  the  individual 
member.  For  the  experts  in  the  field  of  clinical  psychology  or 
those  who  are  mainly  concerned  with  the  therapeutics  of  indi- 
vidual cases,  behavior  difficulties  due  to  individual  differences  may 
be  the  major  concern.  It  surely  is  our  business  to  keep  in  touch 
with  the  findings  of  these  psychologists.  However,  for  the  social 
psychologist,  no  matter  how  unique  and  colorful  these  individual 
factors  may  be,  the  main  concern  first  should  be  the  situational 
determinations  of  group  interactions  and  the  psychological  proper- 
ties that  arise  as  a  consequence  in  the  individual  members.  The 
result  of  multitudes  of  experiments  have  probably  made  it  quite 
clear  by  this  time  that  the  individual  behaves  differently  in  group 
situations.  This  fact  deserves  to  be  noted,  by  the  social  psychologist 
especially,  with  the  same  importance  as  any  fact  of  individual 
differences. 

If  the  adolescent's  efforts  to  achieve  some  stability  for  an  ego 
which  has  been  rendered  more  insecure  and  more  unstable  during 
the  process  of  transition,  if  his  strivings  to  establish  himself  are 
studied  in  terms  of  the  demands  of  his  reference  and  membership 
groups,  perhaps  elaborately  formulated  lists  of  drives  to  account 
for  the  ego-strivings  of  adolescents  will  become  superfluous.  For 
example,  in  one  of  the  recent  accounts  of  adolescent  drives,  we 
find  the  following  items:  "Drive  for  Autonomy,  Drive  for  Social 
Ties  (Social  Acceptance),  Drive  for  Achievement,  Drive  for  Rec- 
ognition, Drive  for  Abasement,  Drive  for  Aggression,  Drive  for 


276  RE-FORMATION  OF  THE  EGO  IN  ADOLESCENCE 

'Succorance,'  Drive  for  Control  (Dominance),  Drive  for  Escape." 
This  impressive  list  certainly  looks  like  a  refined  elaboration  of 
McDougall.  But  by  increasing  the  list  of  drives  or  needs  and 
thus  compartmentalizing  them,  the  functional  relationships  dis- 
played in  behavior  manifestations  aimed  at  achieving  a  desired 
status  are  not  clarified.  We  should  think  rather  that  they  are 
obscured. 

What  an  adolescent,  with  the  unstable  and  more  or  less  transi- 
tory state  of  his  ego  links  and  with  the  more  or  less  confused 
character  of  his  ego  aspirations,  is  striving  to  achieve,  is  to  stabilize 
his  ego-values — to  amount  to  something  or  to  anchor  his  ego 
securely — in  relation  to  his  reference  group,  whatever  this  may  be 
to  him  in  his  particular  social  milieu.  In  order  to  achieve  this,  he 
has  to  and  he  wants  to  identify  himself  with  the  group  or  groups 
in  question.  He  does  his  level  best  to  incorporate  (in  his  ego 
which  is,  we  repeat,  a  genetic  formation)  the  norms  of  the  group, 
whatever  they  may  be  in  his  particular  social  setting.  He  has  to 
and  he  wants  to  conform  to  them  in  his  behavior.  If  conformity 
to  these  norms  is  achieved  by  ruthless  competition  and  individual- 
ism, he  does  his  level  best  to  be  competitive  and  individualistic  to 
the  limits  of  his  capacity.  If  the  norms  of  his  group  put  a  great 
premium  on  being  co-operative,  he  does  his  best  to  be  co-operative. 
Within  the  bounds  of  these  prescribed  directions,  individual 
differences  operate,  sometimes  to  extremes  of  various  kinds  of 
morbidity. 

For  example,  a  Samoan,  a  Zuni,  a  Bathonga  adolescent  [34] 
will  do  his  best,  for  the  sake  of  his  ego,  to  display  individualistic 
behavior  as  little  as  possible.  Perhaps  an  adolescent  in  these  groups 
would  never  dream,  for  the  sake  of  his  ego,  of  exhibiting  behavior 
displayed  by  the  members  of  an  exclusive  fraternity  or  sorority 
oriented  towards  greater  achievements  of  social  distinction  and 
individuality.  In  a  culture  in  which  cooking  is  considered  a 
greater  mark  of  distinction,  a  woman  will  be  less  lured  by  com- 
pliments directed  to,  say,  a  Greer  Garson.12  Anybody  who  is 
familiar  with  behavior  manifestations  in  different  societies  can 

12  An  actual  observation  communicated  to  the  authors  by  Irving  A.  Hallowell 
of  Northwestern  University. 


REFERENCES  277 

give  hundreds  of  examples  of  functional  variations  in  ego-striv- 
ings. In  one,  self-assertiveness  may  be  effective  in  achieving  the  de- 
sired status;  in  another,  submission  and  inconspicuousness  may  be 
more  effective  in  establishing  one's  self  in  his  group.  In  a  society 
in  which  "male  and  female  human  natures"  are  judged  by  double 
standards,  strong  masculine  characteristics  may  raise  one's  appeal 
value  as  a  person.  In  the  same  society,  coyness  and  submissiveness 
may  raise  a  woman's  appeal  value  as  a  person. 

In  short  the  developing  ego,  which  is  a  genetic  formation  itself, 
or  the  re-forming  ego  in  adolescence,  has  to  be  anchored  securely 
and  in  a  relatively  high  standing  in  relation  to  one's  group. 
Whether  this  will  be  achieved  by  being  assertive  or  submissive,  by 
being  individualistic  or  co-operative,  by  the  attainment  of  this  or 
that  sort  of  personal  virtues  and  accomplishments,  will  vary  ac- 
cording to  the  demands  and  pressures  of  one's  own  reference  group 
in  general  and  membership  group  in  particular. 

In  these  chapters,  we  have  given  a  few  functional  variations  of 
adolescent  strivings  to  establish  one's  self  in  his  group.  In  the  next 
chapter  we  shall  cite  some  more  concrete  illustrations.  Scientific 
concepts  are  certainly  the  variables  with  which  investigators  in 
various  fields  deal  with  their  data.  However,  if  the  constructs  or 
concepts  come  in  to  confuse  issues  and  to  compartmentalize  arti- 
ficially the  functional  relationships  of  these  variables,  they  render 
no  scientific  service,  to  say  the  least. 

REFERENCES 

1.  AVERILL,  L.  A.,  'Adolescence:  A  Study  in  the  Teen  Years,  Boston:  Houghton 

Mifflin,  copyright  1936. 

2.  BALDWIN,  B.  T.,  The  Physical  Growth  of  Children  from  Birth  to  Maturity, 

Univ.  Iowa  Stud.:  Stud,  in  Child  Welfare,  vol.  1,  no.  1. 

3.  BAYLEY,  N.,  The  adolescent  growth  study:  III.  Skeletal  X-rays  as  indicators  of 

maturity,  /.  Consult.  PsychoL,  1940,  4,  69-73. 

4. and  R.  TUDDENHAM,  Adolescent  changes  in  body  build,  43rd  yearbook., 

National  Society  for  the  Study  of  Education,  1944,  33-55. 

5.  BLOS,  P.,  The  Adolescent  Personality,  A  Study  of  Individual  Behavior,  New 

York:  Appleton-Century,  copyright  1941. 

6.  BRADLEY,  P.,  The  touch  system  among  coeds,  Real  America,  1934,  4,  31  f. 

7.  BROOKS,  F.  P.,  The  Psychology  of  Adolescence,  Boston:  Riverside  Press,  1929. 

8.  BUHLER,  C.,  The  social  behavior  of  children,  in  Handbook  of  Child  Psychology 

(C.  Murchison,  ed),  .Worcester:  Clark  Univ.,  1933,  2d  ed.,  374-416. 


278  RE-FORMATION  OF  THE  EGO  IN  ADOLESCENCE 

9.  BURKS,  F.  W.,  The  Tugwdl  High  School  Clubhouse  (1936-37),  unpublished, 
Univ.  California,  Inst.  Child  Welfare. 

10.  CAMERON,  W.  ].,  A  Study  of  Social  Development  in  Adolescence,  unpublished, 

Univ.  California,  Inst.  of  Child  Welfare. 

11.  CAMPBELL,  H..  M.,  Sex  differences  obtained  by  the  "Guess  Who"  technique  in 

reputation  assessments  given  and  received  by  adolescent  boys  and  girls,  thesis 
on  file  in  Library,  Univ.  California,  1941. 

12.  CONKLIN,  E.  S.,  Principles  of  Adolescent  Psychology,  New  York:  Holt,  copy- 

right 1935. 

13.  CRESSEY,  P.  G.,  The  Closed  Dance  Halls  in  Chicago,  thesis  on  file  in  Library, 

Univ.  Chicago,  1929. 

14.  DAVIS,  A.,  and  J.  DOLLARD,  Children  of  Bondage,  Washington:  Am.  Council  on 

Education,  1940. 

15.  DAVIS,  K.,  The  sociology  of  parent-youth  conflict,  Am.  Sociol.  Rev.,  1940,  5, 

523-35. 

16.  GARDNER,  G.  E.,  Sex  behavior  of  adolescents  in  wartime,  Ann.  Am.  Acad.  Pol. 

and  Soc.  Sci.f  1944,  236,  60-6. 

17.  GOODENOUGH,  F.  L.,  Developmental  Psychology,  New  York:  Appleton-Century, 

copyright  1945. 

18.  GREULICH,  W.  W.,  Promising  leads  for  research  in  problems  of  adolescence, 

Proc.  Second  Bienn.  Meet,  of  Soc.  Research  in  Child  Development,  Washing- 
ton, 1936,  13-14. 

19.  HEALY,  W.,  Personality  in  Formation  and  Action,  New  York:  Norton,  copy- 

right 1938. 

20.  HOLLINGWORTH,  L.,  The  Psychology  of  the  Adolescent,  New  York:  Applcton, 

copyright  1928. 

2i   Tne  adolescent  child,  in  Handbook  of  Child  Psychology  (C.  Murchison, 

ed.),  Worcester:  Clark  Univ.,  1933,  2d  ed,  882-908. 

22.  HOSKINS,  R.  G.,  The  Tides  of  Life,  New  York:  Norton,  1933. 

23.  JAMESON,  A.  T.,  Psychological  factors  contributing  to  the  delinquency  of  girls, 

/.  Juvenile  Research,  1938,  22,  25-32. 

24.  JANNIS,  J.,  Brothers  under  the  gin,  Real  America,  1934,  4,  30  f. 

25.  JONES,  H.  E.,  Development  in  Adolescence,  New  York:  Appleton-Century, 

copyright  1943. 

26.  KIRKPATRICK,  C.,  A  comparison  of  generations  in  regard  to  attitudes  toward 

feminism,  /.  Genet.  Psychol,  1936,  49,  343-61. 

27. Inconsistency  in  attitudinal  behavior  with  special  reference  to  attitudes 

toward  feminism,  /.  Appl.  Psychol.,  1936,  20,  535-52. 

28.  KITAY,  P.  M.,  A  comparison  of  the  sexes  in  their  attitudes  and  beliefs  about 

women:  a  study  of  prestige  groups,  Sociometry,  1940,  3,  399-407. 

29.  KOMAROVSKY,  M.,  The  Unemployed  Man  and  His  Family,  New  York:  Drydcn, 

1940. 

30.  life,  Teen-age  Girls,  December  11,  1944,  17,  no.  24,  91-9. 
31. Sub-Deb  Clubs,  April  2,  1945, 18,  no.  14,  87-93. 

32. Teen-age  Boys,  June  11,  1945,  18,  no.  24,  91-7. 

33.  LYND,  R.  S.,  and  H.  M.  LYND,  Middletotvn  in  Transition,  New  York:  Harcourt, 

Brace,  copyright  1937. 

34.  MEAD,  M.  (ed.),  Cooperation  and  Competition  Among  Primitive  Peoples,  New 

York:  McGraw-Hill,  1937. 


REFERENCES  279 

35.  MIZANOGLU,  N.,  rA  Study  of  Psychological  Development  During  Adolescence, 

unpublished,  on  file  in  Library,  Univ.  Ankara,  Turkey. 

36.  New  Yor{  World-Telegram,  March  22, 1946. 

37.  OGDEN,  E.,  Regulation  of  circulation  at  adolescence,  Proc.  of  Second  Bienn. 

Meet,  of  Soc.  Research  in  Child  Development,  Washington,  1936,  121-4. 

38.  OGDEN,  M.  B.,  The  Social  Orientation  of  the  Society  Girl,  thesis  on  file  in 

Library,  Univ.  Chicago,  1938. 

39.  SCHWAB,  S.  I.,  and  B.  S.  VEEDER,  The  Adolescent:  His  Conflicts  and  Escapes, 

New  York:  Appleton,  1929. 

40.  SEWARD,  G.  H.,  Cultural  conflict  and  the  feminine  role:  an  experimental  study, 

/.  Soc.  Psychol,  1945,  22,  177-94. 

41.  STOLZ,  H.  R.,  M.  C.  JONES,  and  J.  CHAFFEY,  The  junior  high  school  age,  Univ. 

High  School }.,  1937,  15,  63-72. 

42.  THOMAS,  W.  I.,  The  Unadjusted  Girl,  Boston:  Little,  Brown,  copyright  1927. 

43.  TRYON,  C.  M.,  Evaluations  of  adolescent  personality  by  adolescents,  Monogr. 

Soc.  for  Research  in  Child  Development,  1939,  4,  no.  4. 

44.  Evaluations  of  adolescent  personality  by  adolescents,  in  Child  Behavior 

and  Development  (R.  G.  Barker,  J.  S.  Kounin,  H.  F.  Wright,  eds.),  New 
York:  McGraw-Hill,  copyright  1943,  545-66. 

45.  WHARTON,  E.,  A  Backward  Glance,  New  York:  Appleton-Century,  copyright 

1934. 

46.  WHITE,  R.  W.,  The  personality  of  Joseph  Kidd.    I.  History  of  an  adolescent 

crisis  in  the  development  of  ego-structure,  Character  and  Personality,  1943, 
11,  183-208. 

47.  WILE,  I.  S.,  The  Challenge  of  Adolescence,  New  York:  Greenberg,  copyright 

1939. 

48.  WINCH,  R.  F.,  Social  and  Personality  Characteristics  of  Courtship  Revealed  in 

College  Men,  thesis  on  file  in  Library,  Univ.  Chicago,  1942. 

49.  ZACHRY,  C.  B.,  Emotion  and  Conduct  in  Adolescence,  New  York:  Appleton- 

Century,  copyright  1940. 


CHAPTER  10 

EGO-INVOLVEMENTS  AND  IDENTIFICATIONS  IN 
GROUP  SITUATIONS 

It  has  become  almost  a  truism  by  now  that  group  interaction 
produces  differential  results  in  the  experience  and  behavior  of 
individuals  participating  in  a  group  situation.  Before  we  take  up 
the  problem  of  ego-involvements  and  identifications  in  group  situa- 
tions, we  shall  call  attention  to  the  psychological  properties  gener- 
ated in  group  situations.  Whether  organized  or  not,  when  two  or 
more  (say,  five  hundred  individuals)  interact  in  a  situation,  psy- 
chologically speaking,  we  have  a  group.  The  size  of  the  interact- 
ing group  will,  of  course,  greatly  aflfect  the  characteristics  of  the 
group,  as,  for  example,  its  compellingness  for  the  individual  mem- 
bers. A  group  will  function  differently  depending  on  whether  it 
is  spontaneous  or  organized,  homogeneous  or  heterogeneous. 
However,  without  losing  sight  of  special  problems  these  and  other 
variations  create,  we  can  still  say  that  essentially  the  same  basic 
psychological  principles  are  at  work  in  the  many  variations  of 
group  interaction.1 

At  the  end  of  the  19th  century  and  during  the  first  decades  of 
the  present  century,  sociologists  were  time  and  again  stressing  the 
fact  that  the  individual  in  group  situations  no  longer  experienced 
and  behaved  in  the  same  way  as  he  does  when  in  isolation,  while 
psychologists  in  general  preoccupied  themselves  with  a  search  for 
the  elements  of  the  mind  (sensations)  before  attempting  to  handle 
concrete  problems  of  everyday  life.  Since  sociologists  received 
little  help  from  the  main  body  of  academic  psychology,  they 
tended  to  formulate  their  own  psychological  principles  or  to 
advance  suprapsychological  (supraindividual)  doctrines.  In  the 
writings  of  Durkheim  [16],  Le  Bon  [24],  Blondel  [8],  Ross  [47], 
Martin  [29],  La  Piere  [23],  Blumer  [9],  and  others,  we  find  inter- 


1  For  a  discussion  of  this  point,  see  [56] . 

280 


EGO  IDENTIFICATIONS  IN  GROUP  SITUATIONS  281 

esting  characterizations  of  collective  behavior.  Almost  everyone 
who  wrote  on  collective  behavior  stressed  the  general  fact  that  the 
individual,  when  in  a  group,  becomes  a  "different  person"  than 
he  is  in  isolation  and  may  indulge  in  activities  not  ordinarily 
expected  of  him.  Some  authors  emphasize  the  point  that  the  indi- 
vidual becomes  a  sadistic  beast  when  in  a  collective  situation  (for 
example,  Ross  [47]),  or  releases  his  brakes  altogether  to  regress 
completely  to  the  basic  instinctive  (libidinal)  level  of  psychological 
functioning  (for  example,  Freud).  Others  maintained  that  an 
individual  achieves  the  highest  deeds  of  altruism  while  he  moves 
as  part  and  parcel  of  a  collective  movement  (for  example,  Durk- 
heim  [16]). 

Without  making  a  special  issue  of  the  positive  or  negative  effects 
of  collective  behavior,  we  shall  note  in  passing  that  the  direction 
taken  by  the  activities  of  an  interacting  group  is  determined  by  the 
factors  (motives)  that  are  instrumental  in  bringing  individual 
members  together  and  the  norms  arising  during  the  process  of 
interaction.  For  example,  a  revolutionary  group,  emerging  as  a 
consequence  of  disgust  felt  against  reactionary  forces  and  deter- 
mination to  wipe  them  out,  will  lead  the  individuals  involved  to 
achieve  deeds  of  genuine  selflessness;  a  lynching  party,  instigated 
by  reactionary  ideology  and  groups,  will  commit  beastly  acts; 
while  an  artist  or  nudist  colony  in  the  bohemia  of  sonje  large  city 
may  indulge  in  acts  of  orgiastic  libidinal  satisfactions. 

Starting  especially  with  the  works  of  Walter  Moede  [32]  and 
F.  H.  Allport  [1],  psychologists  studied  differential  group  effects 
on  many  psychological  functions  (for  example,  thinking,  attention, 
feeling).  In  the  excellent  reviews  of  Murphy  and  Murphy  [38]; 
Dashiell  [14];  Murphy,  Murphy,  and  Newcomb  [39];  we  find 
convenient  summaries  of  the  rapidly  accumulating  investigations 
concerned  with  the  effect  of  group  situations.  In  the  genetic 
studies  of  Piaget  and  his  students,  sociometric  studies  started  by 
Moreno,  and  the  experiments  of  Lewin  and  his  students,  the  struc- 
tural, as  contrasted  to  piecemeal  (elementaristic),  properties  of 
group  situations  stand  out  significantly.  We  shall  review  these 
studies  briefly  later  in  this  chapter. 

Impressed  by  the  fact  that  in  actual  social  life  group  norms 
usually  emerge  in  critical  situations,  Sherif  [55]  produced  the  rise 


282  EGO  IDENTIFICATIONS  IN  GROUP  SITUATIONS 

of  group  norms  in  an  experimentally  introduced  ambiguous  and 
unstable  stimulus  situation,  (pp.  52/.)  Cantril  [11],  studying  the 
rise  of  actual  social  movements,  analyzed  the  basic  psychological 
principles  at  work  in  collective  behavior. 

If  we  are  ever  going  to  have  a  social  psychology  of  more  or  less 
lasting  group  situations  and  group  identifications,  we  must  seek 
the  structural  properties  of  group  interaction  and  formation,  group 
products  once  they  are  formed,  their  subsequent  effects  on  the 
behavior  of  individual  members,  and  the  reciprocal  functional 
relationships  between  group  and  individual  members.  If  there 
are  any  consistent  dynamics  involved  in  group  formations  and 
functioning  and  in  the  behavior  of  the  individual  member,  they 
should  be  conceptualized  on  the  basis  of  concrete  observations, 
especially  everyday  life  situations.  This  should  show  us  the  gen- 
eral functional  variables  of  the  group-individual  relationship  in 
any  group  situation  whatever  the  particular  circumstances  may  be 
that  give  rise  to  group  formations  and  whatever  the  particular  type 
of  behavior  thus  determined  or  altered.  Once  this  is  done,  it  will 
be  comparatively  easy  to  study  the  specific  conditions  and  factors 
that  enter  into  the  functioning  of  a  particular  group  and  the 
specific  goals  to  which  it  is  directed. 

In  this  chapter  we  shall  confine  ourselves  to  comparatively  small 
groups  and  to  the  rise  of  norms,  ego-involvements,  and  subsequent 
identifications  in  these  groups.  We  shall  be  concerned  chiefly  with 
spontaneously  formed  small  groups  (gangs).  For  in  such  spon- 
taneously formed  small  groups,  differential  group  behavior,  group 
products,  and  identifications  are  comparatively  easily  traced.  Be- 
cause of  their  relatively  simple  nature,  adolescent  cliques  and  gangs 
are  in  certain  respects  excellent  prototypes  of  group  interaction 
and  formation.  We  can  see  in  them  the  emergence  of  group 
products,  the  determination  of  the  behavior  of  the  individual 
member  by  group  identifications,  the  generation  of  group  loyalties 
and  pressures.  We  should,  however,  make  it  explicit  that  gangs 
are  not  only  adolescent  phenomena.  Drifting  away  from  the  con- 
ventional family  setting  may  occur  before  adolescence,  even  in 
early  childhood,  depending  on  the  degree  to  which  the  child  iden- 
tifies himself  with  his  family  and  other  conventional  groups  or 


EGO  IDENTIFICATIONS  IN  GROUP  SITUATIONS  283 

institutions.  This  identification,  in  turn,  is  regulated  by  complex 
social  and  economic  factors.  However,  as  we  have  seen  in  chapter 
7,  a  certain  degree  of  ego  development  must  take  place  before  a 
child  can  relate  himself  to  any  group.  The  rate  of  this  devel- 
opment is  undoubtedly  affected  by  many  of  the  same  factors 
influencing  the  degree  to  which  he  later  identifies  himself  with 
conventional  groups. 

Already  sociologists  have  amassed  many  impressive  investigations 
of  cliques,  gangs,  isolated  groups,  and  collective  phenomena. 
These  are  full  of  direct  implications  for  the  psychological  con- 
ceptualization of  group  formation  and  of  those  supralocal  (supra- 
individual)  properties  of  group  structures  which  determine  the 
differential  behavior  of  individual  members.  In  their  psychologi- 
cal conceptualizations  of  their  own  data,  sociologists  generally  have 
coined  their  own  "psychologies"  consistent  with  the  ideas  of 
"human  nature"  to  which  their  particular  ideologies  lead  them. 
On  the  other  hand,  psychologists,  just  beginning  to  face  group 
problems  as  psychological  problems,  tend  to  ignore  the  vast  wealth 
of  sociological  data  which  lend  themselves  so  easily  to  psychological 
formulations.  It  seems  that  many  sociologists,  following  the  erro- 
neous traditional  dichotomy  between  the  individual  and  society, 
have  taken  psychology  as  the  apologist  of  the  individual  and  of 
"individualism"  and  have  neglected  to  follow  closely  the  concepts 
psychologists  work  with.  At  the  same  time,  it  is  surprising  that 
most  psychologists  investigating  group  problems  have  not  often 
bothered  to  go  out  of  their  little  worlds  and  examine  the  wealth  of 
material  already  collected  by  sociologists. 

The  main  theme  of  this  chapter  can  be  briefly  summarized  here 
on  the  basis  of  the  sociological  and  psychological  data,  now  con- 
verging in  their  implications.  The  ego  of  the  growing  boy  or  girl 
develops  as  a  consequence  of  various  contacts  with  the  surrounding 
physical  and  socioeconomic  milieu.  This  process  defines  one's 
identity  as  a  person,  anchors  him  in  a  constellation  of  interpersonal 
relationships  in  many  capacities.  His  sense  of  status  and  its  sta- 
bility is  derived  from  this  constellation  of  relationships  interwoven 
during  the  course  of  genetic  development.  He  carries  on  his  daily 
activities  (for  example,  his  strivings  to  make  a  living  and  still  a 
better  living)  as  prescribed  by  these  socially  incorporated  ego- 


284  EGO  IDENTIFICATIONS  IN  GROUP  SITUATIONS 

values.  If  these  ego-values  serve  the  function  of  regulating  his 
activities  in  such  away  that  his  basic  needs  are  more  or  less  satisfied 
and  if  he  feels  secure  in  the  constellation  of  human  relationships 
thus  organized,  he  leads  his  life  as  a  respectable  normal  member  of 
the  group  (subject,  of  course,  to  variations  due  to  individual  differ- 
ences). But,  if  these  values  or  norms  do  not  adequately  satisfy  his 
basic  needs,  or  if  they  produce  conflict  due  to  the  lack  of  integra- 
tion between  the  values  of  different  groups  a  person  identifies  him- 
self with,  then  he  may  strive  restlessly  to  anchor  himself  anew,  or 
he  may  indulge  in  daydreams,  various  sorts  of  abnormal  phan- 
tasies, or  other  "substitutive  activities"  with  their  possible  neurotic 
consequences.  Individuals  who  are  experiencing  basic  depriva- 
tions, who  are  tossing  around  without  a  secure  social  anchoring, 
owing  to  the  contradictory  nature  of  organized  social  relationships, 
may  gravitate  towards  each  other  to  form,  spontaneously,  more  or 
less  well-organized  groups.  And  they  may  then  derive  their  satis- 
factions from  the  activities  and  identifications  such  informally 
organized  groups  make  possible.2 

Most  crucial  and  realistic  observations  on  group  psychology  are 
still  made,  not  by  psychologists,  but  by  observers  who  either  per- 
sonally share  collective  experiences  or  take  a  part  in  the  process  of 
group  transformation.  For,  unlike  psychologists,  these  observers 
cannot  afford  to  make  serious  mistakes.  Concrete  illustrations  of 
this  point  will  be  seen  later  in  relation  to  Makarenko's  work  in 
the  Gorki  Colony  during  the  trying  days  of  the  early  1920's  when 
civil  war  was  raging  in  the  Soviet  Union.  In  the  more  or  less  iso- 
lated situations  of  combat  in  war  where  men  share  misery  and 
dangers  in  the  most  elemental  and  consequential  way,  there  de- 
velop spontaneously  among  them  groups  peculiar  to  them  alone 
in  which  other  men  of  their  own  army  may  not  be  allowed  to 

2  Although  we  shall  not  consider  them  here,  groups  which  arise  because  they 
promise  happiness  to  their  members  through  escapist  solutions  are  good  illustra- 
tions. For  examples  see  [7,  11,  17,  57].  On  the  other  hand,  the  experience  and 
observation  of  misery,  class  contradictions,  social  prejudices  of  all  sorts  which  are 
derivatives  of  these  contradictions  lead  many  people  in  all  countries  to  accept  more 
objectively  valid  explanations  concerning  the  causes  of  this  misery  and  contradic- 
tion. Subsequently,  these  individuals  may  take  a  realistic  firm  stand  in  the  face 
of  many  hardships. 


EGO  IDENTIFICATIONS  IN  GROUP  SITUATIONS  285 

participate.   It  is  just  as  if  the  process  were  regulated  by  an  estab- 
lished norm. 

Among  his  other  keen  observations  of  men  collectively  under 
stress  and  reported  in  words  or  incisive  lines  and  shadings,  Bill 
Mauldin  [31],  who  shared  it  all,  caught  the  major  properties  of 
such  groups: 

While  men  in  combat  outfits  kid  each  other  around,  they  have  a 
sort  of  family  complex  about  it.  No  outsiders  may  join.  Anybody 
who  does  a  dangerous  job  in  this  war  has  his  own  particular  kind  of 
kidding  among  his  own  friends,  and  sometimes  it  doesn't  even  sound 
like  kidding.  Bomber  crews  and  paratroopers  and  infantry  squads 
are  about  the  same  in  that  respect.  If  a  stranger  comes  up  to  a  group 
of  them  when  they  are  bulling,  they  ignore  him.  If  he  takes  it  upon 
himself  to  laugh  at  something  funny  they  have  said,  they  freeze  their 
expressions,  turn  slowly  around,  stare  at  him  until  his  stature  has 
shrunk  to  about  four  inches  and  he  slinks  away,  and  then  they  go 
back  to  their  kidding  again. 

It's  like  a  group  of  prosperous  businessmen  telling  a  risque  joke  and 
then  glaring  at  the  waiter  who  joins  in  the  guffaws.  Combat  people 
are  an  exclusive  set,  and  if  they  want  to  be  that  way,  it  is  their  privi- 
lege. They  certainly  earn  it.  New  men  in  outfits  have  to  work  their 
way  in  slowly,  but  they  are  eventually  accepted.  Sometimes  they 
have  to  change  some  of  their  ways  of  living.  An  introvert  or  a 
recluse  is  not  going  to  last  long  in  combat  without  friends,  so  he 
learns  to  come  out  of  his  shell.  Once  he  has  "arrived"  he  is  pretty 
proud  of  his  clique,  and  he  in  turn  is  chilly  toward  outsiders. 

That's  why,  during  some  of  the  worst  periods  in  Italy,  many  guys 
who  had  a  chance  to  hang  around  a  town  for  a  few  days  after  being 
discharged  from  a  hospital  where  they  had  recovered  from  wounds, 
with  nobody  the  wiser,  didn't  take  advantage  of  it.  They  weren't 
eager  to  get  back  up  and  get  in  the  war,  by  any  means,  and  many  of 
them  did  hang  around  a  few  days.  But  those  who  did  hang  around 
didn't  feel  exactly  right  about  it,  and  those  who  went  right  back  did 
it  for  a  very  simple  reason—not  because  they  felt  that  their  presence 
was  going  to  make  a  lot  of  difference  in  the  big  scheme  of  the  war, 
and  not  to  uphold  the  traditions  of  the  umpteenth  regiment.  A  lot 
of  guys  don't  know  the  name  of  their  regimental  commander.  They 
went  back  because  they  knew  their  companies  were  very  shorthanded, 
and  they  were  sure  that  if  somebody  else  in  their  own  squad  or  section 


286  EGO  IDENTIHCATIONS  IN  GROUP  SITUATIONS 

were  in  their  own  shoes,  and  the  situation  were  reversed,  those  friends 
would  come  back  to  make  the  load  lighter  on  them.  [31,  58  ff.,  all  but 
the  last  italics  ours] 

Further  examples  of  ego-involved  group  loyalties  arising  during 
critical  times  are  reported  in  the  next  chapter. 

Judgments,  perceptions,  and  attitudes  are  referential  affairs,  con- 
sciously or  unconsciously  related  to  a  framework.  If  this  frame 
becomes  unstable  and  conflicting,  judgments  and  attitudes  related 
to  this  frame  are  consequently  apt  to  lose  their  stability  and  inte- 
gration. The  same  is  true  for  the  genetically  formed  ego.  If  we 
lose  our  faith  in  the  persons  or  groups  with  which  we  identify  our- 
selves, or  if  we  are  estranged  from  them  for  various  reasons,  our 
feeling  of  identity  becomes  unstable  and  shaky.  Under  the  cir- 
cumstances, we  are  apt  to  strive  to  anchor  ourselves  more  firmly 
in  a  new  constellation  of  human  relationships.3 

The  case  of  the  "marginal  man,"  with  ambivalent  or  contradic- 
tory group  loyalties,  or  with  experiences  of  successive  acceptance 
and  rejection  by  the  groups  he  is  related  to,  illustrates  our  point 
clearly.  Such  a  man  usually  develops  conflicts  due  to  the  contra- 
dictory components  of  his  loyalties,  or  to  frustrations  arising  from 
his  alternating  acceptance  or  rejection  by  the  groups  he  is  related 
to  in  the  social  order  in  which  these  contradictions  prevail.  In  the 
works  of  Park  and  Miller  [43],  Park  [44],-Stonequist  [58],  Child 

8  The  cases  of  more  or  less  isolated  groups  (for  example,  in  prisons,  concentra- 
tion camps,  or  involuntary  isolation  of  some  sort),  their  formation,  and  the  prod- 
ucts emerging  in  them  may  be  studied  to  exemplify  our  theme.  For  example,  in 
Clemmer's  study  [13]  of  life  in  a  large  prison,  he  notes  that,  because  of  uncer- 
tainty and  confusion  about  the  life  they  have  left  and  because  of  the  impersonal 
prison  routine,  new  inmates  almost  invariably  experience  a  "loss  of  identity"  at 
first.  Some  men  never  recover  from  this  chaotic  state.  However,  the  majority 
come  to  accept  the  prison  code  and  even  anchor  themselves  in  informal  groupings 
to  a  greater  or  lesser  degree.  They  then  identify  themselves  with  the  prison  group, 
and  their  behavior  is  regulated  by  the  code.  Other  members  remain  anchored  in 
persons  or  groups  outside  of  prison  and  manage  to  live  with  a  minimum  of  inner 
conflict,  because  their  behavior  is  judged  by  themselves  in  terms  of  a  reference 
group  outside.  However,  some  men  "have  a  double  or  triple  allegiance,*'  identify- 
ing themselves  with  the  prison  group  in  some  respects,  the  outside  world  in  others. 
It  is  these  prisoners,  the  "betwixt  and  between/'  who  suffer  inner  conflict,  who 
become  "stool  pigeons,"  and  whose  behavior  seems  "confused  and  illogical" 
These  "stool  pigeons"  are  looked  down  upon  scornfully  by  both  inmates  and 
authorities. 


EGO  IDENTIFICATIONS  IN  GROUP  SITUATIONS  287 

[12],  and  others  we  find  interesting  illustrations  of  such  conflict 
situations.  Recently  Hughes  [20]  has  given  a  sociological  account 
of  these  "dilemmas  and  contradictions  of  status,"  which  at  times 
cause  frustrating  experiences  for  the  individuals  caught  in  them. 

Group  identifications.  Studies  of  adolescent  conflicts,  crises, 
identifications,  and  cliques  give  us  important  psychological  clues 
by  means  of  which  we  can  more  adequately  examine  the  intricate 
problems  of  ego-involvements  and  identifications  in  group  situa- 
tions. We  have  seen  how  the  adolescent  boy  or  girl  tosses  in  the 
flux  of  "unsettledness"  and  personal  confusion  with  the  rise  of  the 
sexual  urge  and  the  development  of  other  youthful  aspirations  and 
longings,  with  now  more  intensely  felt  deprivations,  emerging 
interests  and  values.  And  we  will  recall  that  most  of  these  new 
adolescent  yearnings  are  not  sympathetically  shared,  in  general, 
by  the  older  generation  and  are  usually  not  satisfied  in  what  the 
adolescent  regards  as  a  rigid  adult  world  filled  with  restrictions 
and  prohibitions.  The  consequences  are,  in  many  cases,  parent- 
youth  (adult-youth)  conflict,  leading,  for  example,  to:  drifting 
away  ("psychological  weaning")  from  parents  and  other  grown- 
ups, ending  in  actual  running  away  from  the  oppressing  family 
situation  in  some  cases;  crises  of  various  kinds  in  which  the  ado- 
lescent feels  that  nobody  understands  him,  he  is  all  alone  in  an 
unsympathetic  sadistic  world;  or  expressed  or  unexpressed  feelings 
or  acts  of  rebellion  against  different  types  of  authority. 

The  kind  and  the  intensity  of  parent-youth  conflict,  of  psycho- 
logical weaning,  of  crisis  and  rebellion,  and  of  actual  behavior  that 
may  ensue  will  vary  according  to  the  class  structure  of  the  society, 
the  relative  stability  or  instability  of  the  times,  the  rate  of  techno- 
logical changes,  and  the  deprivations  the  particular  adolescent  is 
subject  to  because  of  the  particular  circumstances  of  his  family, 
class,  and  so  on.  A  sociological  analysis  of  certain  of  these  im- 
portant variables  determining  the  amount  and  degree  of  parent- 
youth  conflict  in  a  particular  culture  (as  contrasted  to  universal 
factors,  for  example,  differences  in  age  between  parents  and  chil- 
dren) has  been  made  by  Kingsley  Davis  [15],  Pointing  out  that 
such  conflict  is  minimal  or  inconsequential  in  some  cultures,  Davis 
emphasizes  that,  although  the  physiological  changes  at  adolescence 
are  highly  important  factors,  they  are  universal  and,  consequently, 


288  EGO  IDENTIFICATIONS  IN  GROUP  SITUATIONS 

cannot  in  themselves  account  for  variations  in  the  degree  of  con- 
flict from  one  culture  to  another.  The  four  "complex  variables" 
Davis  analyzes  are:  "(1)  the  rate  of  social  change;  (2)  the  extent 
of  complexity  in  the  social  structure;  (3)  the  degree  of  integration 
in  the  culture;  and  (4)  the  velocity  of  movement  (e.g.,  vertical 
mobility)  within  the  structure  and  its  relation  to  the  cultural 
values."  [535]  Viewing  contemporary  American  culture  in  terms 
of  these  variables,  Davis  comments  that  owing  to  the  rapidity  of 
sociological  change,  not  only  do  the  norms  of  adults  and  youth 
differ  in  content,  but  the  complexity  of  our  society  with  its  lack 
of  integration  leads  to  conflicting  standards,  norms,  and  goals 
"within  the  generations"  (including,  of  course,  those  norms  relat- 
ing to  sex). 

The  feeling  of  insecurity,  of  being  tossed  around,  is  painful,  as 
any  deprivation  is  painful.  It  has  its  limits  of  endurance.  As  a 
result  there  is  a  tendency  to  anchor  one's  self  some  place  where  an 
actual  or  fancied  feeling  of  security  is  provided.  This  anchoring 
may  be  achieved  through  various  kinds  of  identifications  with 
persons  who  have  affective  value  or  who  stand  high  in  one's  eyes; 
or  it  may  be  attained  through  membership  in  some  clique,  gang, 
club,  sect,  select  neighborhood,  or  institution.  In  "casually  pat- 
terned" societies  of  a  high  degree  of  development  and  differentia- 
tion, to  borrow  a  concept  used  by  Robert  Lynd  in  his  provocative 
book,  Knowledge  for  What?  The  Place  of  Social  Science  in  Ameri- 
can Culture  [28],  efforts  to  get  one's  self  anchored  may  be  directed 
to  contradictory  and  escapist  orientations.  These  contradictory 
identifications  and  affiliations  may  develop  into  absurd  proportions. 
Instead  of  giving  a  consistent  feeling  of  security,  they  may  only 
produce  further  conflicts.4 

Youngsters  who  are  striving,  sometimes  desperately,  to  get  out 
of  a  psychological  crisis  situation,  to  find  for  themselves  an  atmos- 
phere of  experience  and  action  sympathetic  to  and  in  harmony 
with  their  yearnings  and  aspirations,  or  to  satisfy  keenly  felt  dep- 
rivations, spontaneously  form  adolescent  groups  (cliques,  gangs) 
with  kindred  souls.  The  structural  properties  of  such  groups — 

4  For  some  accentuated  illustrations  of  this  point,  see  the  references  on  the 
"marginal  man"  and  ch.  11. 


THE  STRUCTURAL  PROPERTIES  OF  GROUPS  289 

such  as  degree  of  solidarity,  integration,  degree  of  exclusiveness 
of  loyalty  demanded— will  vary  according  to  the  extent  to  which 
each  individual  member  is  driven  to  cast  his  own  personal  lot 
with  a  group  as  determined  by  the  locality  in  which  he  lives,  the 
degree  of  mobility  possible,  economic  class,  and  ethnic  affiliations. 
For  economic  class  and  ethnic  affiliations  are  major  determinants 
of  significant  identifications  and  anchorages  or  of  the  lack  of  them, 
especially  in  bourgeois  societies  where  rigid  prejudices  prevail  (as 
seen  in  "social  distance"  studies). 

The  goals  towards  which  clique  or  gang  activities  will  be  di- 
rected vary  according  to  the  character  of  the  major  deprivations, 
the  character  of  the  feeling  of  not  belonging,  and  the  aspirations 
of  the  individuals  in  the  gang  or  clique.  A  certain  gang  of  the 
type  most  frequently  studied  by  sociologists  may  engage  particu- 
larly in  pilfering,  "jack-rolling"  or  stealing.  Another  gang,  whose 
members  are  economically  above  the  subsistence  level,  may  special- 
ize in  orgiastic  or  other  sexual  activities,  a  third  gang  or  clique 
may  be  noted  for  its  acts  of  "distinction"  in  assuring  for  its  mem- 
bers an  envied  status  in  their  social  world.  School  gangs  or  cliques 
of  the  latter  kind  have  not  attracted  so  much  attention,  perhaps 
because  they  do  not  stand  out  as  spectacular.  As  we  saw  in  the  last 
chapter,  college  fraternities  and  sororities  almost  serve  as  a  proto- 
type of  groups  engaging  in  such  activities. 

THE  STRUCTURAL  PROPERTIES  OF  GROUPS 

Studies  of  the  spontaneous  formations  of  gangs  and  the  subse- 
quent rise  of  appropriate  norms  (codes)  and  roles  which  define  the 
relative  status  of  individual  members,  give  us  excellent  informa- 
tion concerning  the  character  of  group  structures,  identifications, 
and  loyalties,  and  their  determination  of  the  behavior  of  the  mem- 
bers. The  material  on  gangs  is  especially  helpful  in  formulating 
psychological  generalizations,  since  sociologists  have  published 
elaborate  studies  on  the  formation  and  functioning  of  gangs,  in- 
cluding case  histories  of  individual  members.  The  group  proper- 
ties, individual  identifications,  and  behavior  in  gangs  found  here 
give  us,  as  we  have  said,  the  essential  prototype  of  any  group  or 
group  behavior. 


290  EGO  IDENTIFICATIONS  IN  GROUP  SITUATIONS 

In  these  gang  studies,  a  cardinal  fact  concerning  the  behavior 
of  individual  members  in  any  collective  situation  stands  out  in  high 
relief:  the  fact  that,  once  an  individual  identifies  himself  with  a 
group  and  its  collective  actions,  his  behavior  is,  in  a  major  way, 
determined  by  the  direction  of  the  group  action,  whatever  this 
direction  may  be,  good  or  bad,  constructive  or  destructive.  Once 
an  individual  identifies  himself  with  the  group,  whether  this  group 
is  a  clique,  gang,  club,  fraternity,  sect,  or  political  organization,  his 
ego  becomes  so  involved  that  he  is  apt  to  feel  a  loss  of  personal 
status  if  he  does  not  keep  pace  with  group  activity.  His  social 
status  as  he  feels  it  consists  largely  of  a  constellation  of  these  group 
identifications,  whatever  the  specific  character  and  level  in  the 
particular  scheme  of  social  stratification  may  be.  Deviations  and 
injuries  inflicted  on  his  identifications,  either  by  himself  or  by 
others,  are  usually  experienced  as  a  personal  loss  of  prestige.  Thus 
a  gang  member  acquires  his  real  social  status  in  his  gang,  even 
though  he  may  have  learned  by  heart  all  the  prevailing  norms  of 
society  at  large. 

This  fact  is  nicely  pointed  out  by  Zorbaugh:  [63] 

The  church,  the  school,  and  the  occupational  group,  then,  as  well 
as  the  newspaper,  play  no  intimate  role  in  the  local  life  of  the  Near 
North  Side  [of  Chicago],  .  .  .  The  Gold  Coast  has  its  clubs;  intimate 
groups  gather  in  "village"  studios;  the  foreign  areas  have  numerous 
lodges  and  mutual  benefit  societies;  the  slum  has  its  "gangs."  Even 
in  the  rooming-house  area,  where  group  life  is  at  a  minimum,  occa- 
sional cults  and  sects  spring  up,  and  every  pool  hall  and  cigar  store 
has  about  it  a  nebulous  group.  And  these  groups  may  play  an  enor- 
mously important  role  in  the  lives  of  their  members.  [63, 192] 

In  Thrasher's  work  we  have  an  account  of  hundreds  of  gangs 
studied  in  the  early  1920's.  Thrasher's  book  is  a  mine  of  rich  data 
for  the  social  psychologist.  It  is  full  of  material  which  shows  the 
process  of  group  formation  under  well-defined  economic  and 
social  situations,  the  structural  properties  of  groups,  and  the  rise 
of  appropriate  norms  (codes)  that  regulate  the  behavior  of  indi- 
vidual members  to  such  an  important  degree. 

Before  we  analyze  the  structural  properties  of  "gangs,"  it  is 
important  to  note  that,  no  matter  what  kind  of  "deviant"  activities 


THE  STRUCTURAL  PROPERTIES  OF  GROUPS  291 

gang  boys  engage  in,  they  are  generally  wholesome  and  normal 
human  beings,  subject  only  to  the  usual  individual  differences 
found  in  any  school,  Sunday  school,  or  club  population.  Thrasher 
[60]  states  that 

Aside  from  their  usual  lack  of  adjustment  to  the  formal  demands 
of  conventional  social  codes,  however,  gang  boys  ...  are  not  morbid 
or  psychopathic  as  is  sometimes  the  case  with  boys  who  are  subject 
to  the  repressions  and  punishments  of  a  more  artificial  situation.  [60, 
172] 

Other  investigators  substantiate  the  fact:  we  see  it  in  Clifford 
Shaw's  Brothers  in  Crime  [52] ; 5  in  Whyte's  Street  Corner  Society 
[62];  and  Zorbaugh's  The  Gold  Coast  and  the  Slum  [63].  Gangs 
are  formed  spontaneously  to  serve  the  function  of  a  social  institu- 
tion, to  secure  a  status  and  a  social  identity  for  youngsters  not 
genuinely  provided  with  such  an  identity  by  society  at  large.  And 
perhaps  even  more  important,  gangs  regulate  and  encourage  ac- 
tivities which  satisfy  basic  human  needs,  because  families  and 
communities  have  failed  to  provide  many  children  with  the  means 
of  relieving  major  deprivations. 

Gangs  represent  the  spontaneous  effort  of  boys  to  create  a  society  for 
themselves  where  none  adequate  to  their  needs  exists.  [60,  37] 

It  offers  a  substitute  for  what  society  fails  to  give;  and  it  provides  a 
relief  from  suppression  and  distasteful  behavior.  [60,  38] 

This  comes  out  again  and  again  in  representative  studies.  For 
example,  we  see  in  Zorbaugh's  conclusion  [63]  how  restrictions 
imposed  by  privileged  classes  to  maintain  class  lines  encourage 
gang  formation. 

The  child  cannot  live  and  conform  in  both  social  worlds  at  the 
same  time.  The  family  and  colony  are  defined  for  him  in  his  Ameri- 
can contacts  by  such  epithets  as  "dago,"  "wop,"  "foreign,"  and  the  like. 
He  feels  the  loss  of  status  attached  to  his  connection  with  the  colony. 
In  his  effort  to  achieve  status  in  the  American  city  he  loses  his  rapport 
with  family  and  community.  Conflicts  arise  between  the  child  and 
his  family.  Yet  by  virtue  of  his  race,  his  manner  of  speech,  the  neces- 

5  The  clinical  summaries  contained  in  this  book  and  made  by  the  psychiatrist, 
Harold  Hanson  [ch.  13]  are  particularly  to  the  point. 


292  EGO  IDENTIFICATIONS  IN  GROUP  SITUATIONS 

sity  of  living  in  the  colony,  and  these  same  definitive  epithets,  he  is 
excluded  from  status  and  intimate  participation  in  American  life, 
Out  of  this  situation,  as  we  have  already  seen,  arises  the  gang,  afford- 
ing the  boy  a  social  world  in  which  he  finds  his  only  status  and  rec- 
ognition. But  it  is  by  conforming  to  delinquent  patterns  that  he 
achieves  status  in  the  gang.  And  every  boy  in  Little  Hell  is  a  mem- 
ber of  a  gang.  [63,  176  f.f  italics  ours] 

Thrasher  [60]  begins  his  study  by  giving  socioeconomic  account 
of  the  areas  particularly  conducive  to  the  formation  of  gangs 
(gangland).  He  writes  that 

The  gang  is  almost  invariably  characteristic  of  regions  that  are 
interstitial  to  the  more  settled,  more  stable,  and  better  organized  por- 
tions of  the  city.  The  central  tripartite  empire  of  the  gang  occupies 
what  is  often  called  "the  poverty  belt"— -a  region  characterized  by 
deteriorating  neighborhoods,  shifting  populations,  and  the  mobility 
and  disorganization  of  the  slum.  [60,  22] 

It  is  against  this  situational  background  that  the  activities  of  such 
gangs  become  significant.  The  specific  activities  of  different  gangs 
may  vary,  as  Thrasher  points  out.  The  situational  determinants  in 
each  case  must  be  specifically  studied.6 

Thrasher,  like  some  other  sociologists  in  the  field,  tends  to 
consider  gangs  as  cases  of  disorganization.  This  seems  to  us  a 
moralistic  view  of  the  subject.  As  Whyte  recently  noted,  activities 
of  slum  areas  and  those  of  gangs  have  an  internal  organization  of 
their  own  while  they  last.7  In  Whyte's  words: 

It  is  customary  for  the  sociologist  to  study  the  slum  district  in  terms 
of  "social  disorganization"  and  to  neglect  to  see  that  an  area  such  as 
Cornerville  has  a  complex  and  well-established  organization  of  its 
own.  I  was  interested  in  that  organization.  I  found  that  in  every 
group  there  was  a  hierarchical  structure  of  social  relations  binding  the 

6  The  almost  universal  formation  of  gangs  among  young  boys  in  certain  areas 
of  the  population  formerly  has  led  certain  authors  to  posit  such  conceptions  as  a 
"gang  instinct"  (see,  for  example,  J.  Puffer  [46]).    But  sociologists  like  Thrasher 
and  Shaw  who  are  intimately  acquainted  with  gangs  have  dismissed  such  untenable 
psychological  positions  in  favor  of  situational  (environmental)  explanations. 

7  We  say  "while  they  last"  because  the  impact  of  the  established  society,  which 
contributed  to  the  gang's  formation,  at  the  same  time  impinges  upon  it  in  the 
direction  of  its  dissolution.   We  shall  comment  on  this  point  later. 


THE  STRUCTURAL  PROPERTIES  OF  GROUPS  293 

individuals  to  one  another  and  that  the  groups  were  also  related 
hierarchically  to  one  another.  Where  the  group  was  formally  organ- 
ized into  a  political  club,  this  was  immediately  apparent,  but  for 
informal  groups  it  was  no  less  true.  While  the  relations  in  such 
groups  were  not  formally  prescribed,  they  could  be  clearly  observed 
in  the  interactions  of  individuals.  [62,  viii,  italics  ours] 

As  we  shall  see  presently,  adolescent  boys,  particularly  those  who 
experience  economic  stress  and  deprivation,  who  are  actually  (if 
not  legally)  denied  the  opportunity  to  identify  themselves  organ- 
ically with  society  at  large,  cannot  help  but  gravitate  to  each  other 
in  their  common  destiny  to  form  spontaneously  more  or  less  or- 
ganized groups  which  provide  them  with  some  kind  of  status, 
some  means  of  satisfying  their  youthful  deprivations.  If  these 
same  boys  could  identify  themselves  with  a  group  that  held  oppo- 
site values  and  used  other  devices  to  achieve  the  same  ends,  they 
would  have  formed  their  loyalties  and  oriented  their  activities 
accordingly. 

In  chapter  7  we  pointed  out  certain  important  implications 
which  contact  with  age-mate  groups  had  on  the  process  of  ego 
development.  Thrasher  comments: 

The  majority  of  gangs  develop  from  the  spontaneous  play-group. 
[60,  29] 

In  the  light  of  our  earlier  discussion,  this  fact  acquires  particular 
significance.  More  recently,  Whyte  [61]  further  substantiated 
Thrasher's  observation: 

The  clique  structure  arises  out  of  the  habitual  association  of  mem- 
bers, over  a  long  period  of  time.  The  nuclei  of  most  gangs  can  be 
traced  back  to  early  boyhood  years  when  living  close  together  provided 
the  first  opportunities  of  social  contacts.  [61] 

These  observations  show  in  a  concrete  way  the  effect  of  spontane- 
ous age-mate  contacts  and  games  on  the  developing  social  identity 
of  children.  It  is  immaterial  for  our  argument,  of  course,  whether 
all  gangs  develop  from  early  childhood  contacts,  or  whether  many 
of  them  are  products  of-  the  adolescent  age  level.  Clifford  Shaw 
reports  several  concrete  cases  of  gangs  which  started  when  young- 
sters exchanged  stories  of  exciting  experiences  and  began  to  or- 


294  EGO  IDENTIFICATIONS  IN  GROUP  SITUATIONS 

ganize  various  exploits.  These  interactions  tend  to  produce  a 
group  structure  which  varies  in  its  stability  according  to  the 
situation. 

In  the  process  of  group  (gang)  formation,  the  relative  standing 
of  one  individual  to  another  becomes  defined.  Each  member 
acquires  a  relative  status  with  appropriate  functions  of  authority 
and  responsibility.  The  leader  himself  emerges  in  the  process  of 
interaction  in  diverse  group  activities.  Thrasher  points  out: 

There  is  a  process  of  selection  in  the  gang,  as  a  result  of  the  struggle 
for  status,  whereby  the  ultimate  position  of  each  individual  is  deter- 
mined. The  result  of  this  process  depends  largely  upon  the  individual 
differences — both  native  and  acquired — which  characterize  the  mem- 
bers of  the  group.  [60,  334] 

Whyte,  who  lived  in  the  slums  and  intimately  associated  with 
gangs,  formulated  in  a  clear-cut  way  the  crystallization  of  a  relative 
role  or  status  for  each  member,  as  well  as  shifts  of  status  with 
changing  situations: 

Each  member  of  the  corner  gang  has  his  own  position  in  the  gang 
structure.  Although  positions  may  remain  unchanged  over  long 
periods  of  time,  they  should  not  be  conceived  in  static  terms.  To  have 
a  position  means  that  the  individual  has  a  customary  way  of  interact- 
ing with  other  members  of  the  group.  When  the  pattern  of  inter- 
actions changes,  the  positions  change.  [62,  262 /.] 

On  the  basis  of  extensive  observations  of  hundreds  of  gangs, 
Thrasher  states: 

While  it  may  sometimes  be  true  that  a  gang  forms  about  a  leader, 
the  reverse  is  generally  true:  the  gang  forms  and  the  leader  emerges 
as  the  result  of  interaction.  [60,  351] 

Certainly  individual  differences  in  various  psychological  charac- 
teristics of  the  members,  their  relatively  high  or  low  status  in  the 
group,  their  superiority  in  diverse  abilities  may  secure  a  particular 
member  leadership  in  the  gang.  Thrasher  mentions  such  charac- 
teristics as  physical  strength,  athletic  prowess,  gameness,  quickness 
and  firmness  of  decision,  and  imagination  as  qualities  which  deter- 
mine the  leadership  of  a  particular  member  in  a  particular  gang. 
Which  one  of  these  or  other  qualities  will  help  a  person  gain 


THE  STRUCTURAL  PROPERTIES  OF  GROUPS  295 

leadership  in  a  gang  is  situationally  determined  by  the  particular 
pattern  of  activities  the  gang  goes  in  for.  As  Thrasher  states: 

The  marks  of  leadership  vary  from  gang  to  gang.  The  type  of  boy 
who  can  lead  one  gang  may  be  a  failure  or  have  a  distinctly  subordi- 
nate role  in  another.  .  .  .  Physical  and  athletic  prowess,  which  stand 
the  leader  in  such  good  stead  in  most  gangs,  for  example,  v/ould  not 
be  valued  in  the  following  type  of  group.  [60,  344] 

Then  he  cites  the  case  of  "The  Bandits"  who,  while  carrying  on 
"delinquent"  activities,  engage  as  a  group  in  such  activities  as 
"social  and  folk  dancing  with  girls."  In  another  example  he  notes 
that 

A  hunchback  was  a  very  successful  leader  of  a  gang  of  healthy  boys. 
[60,  350] 

Studies  on  personality  traits,  such  as  aggressiveness  or  submissive- 
ness,  acquire  concrete  meaning  only  when  the  situational  factors 
in  any  group  situation  are  given  due  consideration. 

Leadership  in  a  group  is  by  no  means  an  accidental  or  arbitrary 
affair.  The  status  of  the  leader  is  exercised  not  simply  by  virtue  of 
some  personality  trait,  but  rather  because  the  leader  lives  up  to 
certain  expectations  demanded  by  the  pattern  of  established  rela- 
tionships. As  Thrasher  states, 

The  leader  has  considerable  power  over  his  subordinates  so  long  as 
he  does  not  abuse  it.  [60,  292,  italics  ours] 

Whyte  [61]  points  out  that  the  higher  a  member's  status  is,  the 
stricter  the  expectations  are  for  the  fulfillment  of  his  responsibilities 
and  obligations. 

Not  all  corner  boys  live  up  to  their  obligations  equally  well,  and  this 
factor  partly  accounts  for  the  differentiation  in  status  among  the  men. 
The  man  with  a  low  status  may  violate  his  obligations  without  much 
change  in  position.  [61,  657] 

But  this  is  not  the  case  for  members  with  high  status,  including  the 
leader.  The  leader,  as  Thrasher  says,  "goes  where  others  fear  to 
go"  in  the  exploits  of  the  gang. 

The  number  of  members  in  the  gang  may  range  from  two  to 
several  hundred.  It  is  interesting  to  note  that  usually  the  member- 


296  EGO  IDENTIFICATIONS  IN  GROUP  SITUATIONS 

ship  does  not  exceed  limits  which  will  make  interpersonal  contacts 
difficult  or  direct  face-to-face  group  pressure  on  the  members  im- 
possible. For  example,  out  of  895  gangs  Thrasher  examined  from 
this  point  of  view,  538  gangs  had  membership  within  the  range  of 
10  to  20  members  each. 

The  specific  variations  in  the  particular  situations  that  give  rise 
to  gangs  and  the  variations  in  the  aspirations  produce  gangs  with 
different  characteristics. 

No  two  gangs  are  just  alike.  The  cases  investigated  present  an 
endless  variety  of  forms,  and  every  one  is  in  some  sense  unique. 
[60,45] 

Gangs  specialize  in  certain  types  of  activity: 

Gangs  which  develop  specialized  structures  and  codes  for  the  fur- 
therance of  some  interest  of  their  own  may  be  regarded  as  functional 
types.  Thus,  groups  are  organized  around  such  dominant  interests  as 
junking,  sex,  picking  pockets,  stealing,  athletics,  gambling,  or  some 
special  type  of  crime.  In  each  case  they  develop  their  own  technique. 
[60,  297] 

These  different  groups  start  spontaneously  around  a  more  or  less 
diffuse  nucleus  and  become  structured  and  organized  in  various 
degrees  as  time  goes  on. 

All  sorts  of  collective  products  emerge  in  the  process  of  inter- 
action in  group  activities  and  experiences.  The  new  group  prod- 
ucts further  strengthen  the  group  structure  and  solidarity,  re- 
enforcing  group  pressure  on  the  individual  members.  Gang 
names,  such  as  "Dirty  Dozen,"  "Lilies  of  the  Valley,"  "Shielders," 
"Hudson  Dusters,"  "Yakey  Yakes,"  "Five  Points,"  "Vultures," 
"Forty  Thieves,"  add  to  the  feeling  of  group  belongingness.8 
Gang  members  are  called  by  special  nicknames  assigned  according 
to  their  relative  positions  in  the  group.  Thrasher  says: 

Personalities  are  recognized  by  the  names  applied  to  the  members 
of  the  gang.  Individual  peculiarities,  which  have  an  important  effect 
in  determining  status,  are  likely  to  give  color  to  the  boy's  whole  per- 
sonality. He  is  named  accordingly,  and  his  name  often  indicates  the 
esteem  in  which  he  is  held  by  the  group.  [60,  339  f.] 

•These  arc  names  of  actual  gangs  taken  from  [60,  275 /.,  281] . 


THE  STRUCTURAL  PROPERTIES  OF  GROUPS  297 

In  the  course  of  time,  the  gang  develops  definite  boundaries  as 
its  area  of  activity.  Psychologically,  this  area  is  appropriated  as 
the  gang's  own  area  for  operation.  Violations  of  this  appropriation 
by  some  other  gang  are  on  occasion  reacted  to  violently. 

Just  as  among  nations  borderline  disputes  sometimes  precipitate 
disastrous  wars,  so  gangs  may  be  mobilized  and  led  to  battle  on  the 
same  issue.  [60,  726] 

Time  and  again,  Thrasher  calls  attention  to  the  fact  that  group 
solidarity,  the  "we"  feeling,  is  strengthened  through  outside  re- 
sistances, collective  fights,  and  alliances  with  other  gangs.  In  some 
cases,  this  outside  resistance  may  come  from  adults.  Thrasher 
quotes  a  gang  member: 

''This  desire  to  escape  family  supervision  marked  the  beginning  of 
our  feeling  of  solidarity.  Our  first  loyalties  were  to  protect  each  other 
against  our  parents."  [60,  31] 

Similar  to  the  feelings  of  belongingness  in  any  group,  and  the 
development  and  delineation  of  attitudes  towards  the  "in-group" 
and  "out-group"  members,  attitudes  develop  which  define  co- 
operative and  sympathetic  in-group  relationships,  various  degrees 
of  prejudice  and  of  social  distance  towards  outsiders. 

All  systematic  investigators  like  Thrasher,  Shaw,  Zorbaugh, 
Landesco,  and  Whyte  have  observed  that  appropriate  norms  arise. 
These,  in  turn,  regulate  the  formation  of  the  attitudes  and  the 
subsequent  behavior  of  the  members.  In  Thrasher's  words: 

Every  gang  tends  to  develop  its  own  code  of  conduct,  of  which  its 
members  are  more  or  less  aware  and  which  may  be  more  or  less 
rigidly  enforced  upon  them.  The  code  of  the  gang  is  in  part  reflected 
from  the  patterns  of  behavior  in  its  own  social  world,  in  part  the 
result  of  the  development  of  primary  group  sentiments,  and  in  part 
the  product  of  the  individual  group  in  its  own  special  environment. 
[60,  284] 

In  a  similar  way,  group  slogans  arise  which  formulate  gang  prac- 
tices in  a  short-cut  way.  Expressive  words  become  standardized 
for  the  gang,  have  special  meaning  to  the  members. 


298  EGO  IDENTIFICATIONS  IN  GROUP  SITUATIONS 

The  isolated  life  of  gangland  leads  to  the  development  of  a  distinct 
universe  of  discourse.  .  .  .  Like  its  morality,  this  argot,  too,  follows  to 
some  extent  patterns  in  its  own  social  world.  ...  [60,  266  f.] 

As  in  any  social  group  of  any  degree  of  solidarity,  these  norms 
are  inculcated  in  the  individual  members  in  the  very  process  of 
interaction,  generating  feelings  of  belongingness  and  loyalty. 
Also,  as  in  any  social  group,  various  sorts  of  pressures,  correctives 
(such  as  applause  and  ridicule),  and  punishments  are  applied  to 
keep  the  attitudes  and  behavior  of  the  members  in  line. 

Opinion  in  the  gang  manifests  its  pressure  in  the  variety  of  mech- 
anisms through  which  group  control  is  exerted,  such  as  applause,  pre- 
ferment and  hero  worshipping  as  well  as  ridicule,  scorn,  and  ostracism. 
...  In  the  gang  the  member  who  has  broken  the  code  may  be  sub- 
jected to  a  beating  or  in  extreme  cases  may  be  marked  for  death. 
[60,  297 /..italics  ours] 

Deviations  from  group  behavior  are  thus  reacted  to  by  the  group 
as  a  whole  with  varying  degrees  of  severity.  Since  "squealing"  is 
the  worst  deviation,  in  some  cases  it  is  punishable  by  death.  On 
the  positive  side,  group  identification  generates  at  times  the  highest 
degree  of  co-operative  and  sympathetic  tendencies  within  the  de- 
linquent gang.  There  exists  within  the  gang  "a  sort  of  brother- 
hood and  mutual  kindliness.  This  manifests  itself  in  many  forms 
of  self-sacrifice.  If  a  member  is  in  serious  danger  the  rest  will 
spare  no  pains  to  save  his  life.  One  boy  will  sometimes  undergo 
severe  hardships  to  aid  another."  [290] 

After  a  boy  becomes  a  good  member  of  the  gang,  his  personal 
identity  is  linked  to  that  of  the  group.  His  status  in  the  gang  is, 
in  a  major  way,  his  status  as  he  experiences  it.  For  it  is  in  the  gang 
interaction  that  he  lives  his  life  intimately  and  intensely.  When 
his  ties  with  the  gang  are  strained,  he  may  feel  he  is  losing  his 
ground.  Thrasher  aptly  puts  it: 

Any  standing  in  the  group  is  better  than  none,  and  there  is  always 
the  possibility  of  improving  one's  status.  Participation  in  gang  activi- 
ties means  everything  to  the  boy.  It  not  only  defines  for  him  his 
position  in  the  only  society  he  is  greatly  concerned  with,  but  it  becomes 
the  basis  of  his  conception  of  himself.  [60,  332,  italics  ours] 


TOE  STRUCTURAL  PROPERTIES  OF  GROUPS  299 

Consequently,  his  feeling  of  personal  security  is  derived  from  gang 
membership.  This  feeling  of  security  "tends  to  remove  the  qualms 
that  might  well  arise  in  an  individual  embarking  upon  some  peril- 
ous undertaking  on  his  own  account!'  [296,  italics  ours]  Here  we 
see  the  differences,  even  qualitative  differences,  between  the  indi- 
vidual alone,  or  in  one  situation,  and  the  same  individual  in  a 
group  with  which  he  strongly  identifies  himself.  Under  the  pres- 
sure of  the  group  situation,  the  individual's  social  identity  may  be 
transformed. 

Thrasher  cites  many  concrete  cases  of  such  differential  group 
behavior.  In  the  report  on  one  gang  by  an  ex-member  we  will  see 
(p.  303)  how  a  member  of  a  gang  where  toughness  was  at  a  pre- 
mium dropped  his  ruffian  ways  and  was  always  quiet  and  courte- 
ous when  he  went  out  on  the  sly  with  his  girl.  Here  is  another 
example: 

We  have  one  case  of  an  Italian  boy,  R ,  who  in  the  gang  can 

always  be  counted  upon  to  respond  to  an  appeal  for  the  best  for  him- 
self and  the  gang.  Outside  the  gang  his  record  has  not  been  so  satis- 
factory. In  the  gang  he  is  stimulated  by  group  appreciation;  while 
without  he  is  not.  [60,  295] 

Group  loyalty  may  at  times  dominate  over  serious  cases  of  per- 
sonal injury,  as  we  shall  see  shortly  (p.  301)  in  the  case  of  a 
gang  member  who  refused  to  squeal  on  another  member  who  had 
cut  his  head  open  by  a  blow  with  a  lead  pipe.  Individuals  in 
group  situations  sometimes  become  so  personally  involved  in  the 
collective  situation  that  they  do  not  isolate  themselves  as  individ- 
uals to  figure  out  what  is  most  advantageous  to  them  personally. 

Whatever  we  did,  we  knew  would  be  done  as  a  group.  At  the  end 
of  the  season  Steve  suggested  that  we  enlist  as  a  body  (the  whole 
team),  and  leave  at  once.  Some  wanted  to  join  the  marines;  some 
the  army;  but  a  vote  decided  on  the  navy.  In  order  to  stay  together, 
we  went  to  the  Great  Lakes  Naval  Training  Station  and  enlisted  as 
a  group  of  apprentice  seamen,  even  though  some  of  us  could  have 
received  ratings  had  we  been  willing  to  quit  the  others.  [60, 188] 

All  of  these  facts  indicate  the  inadequacy  of  any  approach  to 
personality  which  posits  unchanging  personality  traits,  especially 
those  concerned  with  social  characteristics,  without  due  regard  to 


300  EGO  IDENTIFICATIONS  IN  GROUP  SITUATIONS 

the  determination  of  behavior  by  situational  factors  generated  in 
any  collective  interaction  in  a  psychologically  lawful  way.  These 
psychological  products  emerging  in  the  process  of  gang  formation 
and  conduct  are  amply  illustrated  by  Thrasher.  His  account  of 
"The  Dirty  Dozen"  gang,  which  he  characterizes  as  a  fairly  typical 
group,  will  give  us  concrete  evidence  of  the  structural  properties, 
emergent  products,  and  subsequent  attitudes  and  identifications 
found  in  a  group.  This  report  was  made  by  a  former  associate  of 
the  gang. 

The  Dirty  Dozen  began  merely  as  the  result  of  a  dozen  or  more 
fellows  (from  sixteen  to  twenty-two  years  of  age)  meeting  casually 
on  a  street  corner  at  the  entrance  of  one  of  Chicago's  parks  and  later 
on  in  "Mike's"  poolroom  a  short  distance  away.  Most  of  the  boys 
were  loafers,  who  spent  their  time  swimming,  playing  baseball  and 
football,  shooting  craps,  or  sitting  around  and  talking.  They  liked 
brawls  and  fights,  and  the  gang  helped  to  satisfy  these  wants  with  less 
personal  discomfort  than  might  occur  if  one  fellow  alone  started  hos- 
tilities or  tried  to  steal  something.  Of  their  various  activities,  some 
form  of  conflict  seems  to  have  been  the  chief. 

There  was  war  between  the  gang  and  the  police,  for  even  though 
the  latter  did  not  always  have  any  particular  offense  for  which  the 
fellows  were  wanted,  they  did  try  to  break  up  the  group  whenever  it 
congregated  on  the  corner. 

The  gang  as  a  whole  often  came  into  direct  conflict  with  other 
gangs.  One  night  at  the  old  Imperial  Theater,  the  Dirty  Dozen  found 
themselves  seated  opposite  the  "Chi"  gang,  their  rival  in  football  and 
baseball.  During  the  show,  which  was  poor  vaudeville,  the  fellows 
started  to  hurl  remarks  at  each  other.  The  verbal  conflict  grew  into  a 
near-riot,  which  continued  until  the  police  came. 

The  Dirty  Dozen,  however,  was  capable  of  collective  action  against 
other  enemies  than  rival  gangs.  One  night  while  the  race  riots  of  1919 
were  at  their  height,  the  gang,  armed  with  revolvers,  blackjacks,  and 
knives,  started  out  to  get  the  "niggers." 

At  Thirty-fifth  and  State  streets,  five  miles  or  more  from  their  own 
territory,  and  after  some  preliminary  skirmishes,  "Shaggy"  Martin 
threw  the  trolley  of  a  street  car  filled  with  colored  people.  The  rest  of 
the  gang,  which  had  increased  to  about  twenty  by  this  time,  piled  on, 
"Shaggy,"  who  was  left  alone  at  the  back  to  hold  the  trolley-rope, 


THE  STRUCTURAL  PROPERTIES  OF  GROUPS  301 

was  standing  there  with  it  in  one  hand  and  a  billy  in  the  other  when 
a  colored  woman  slashed  him  across  the  heart  with  a  razor.  Then 
someone  hit  her,  and  another  fellow  "got"  her  husband. 

Shaggy  died  in  the  patrol  on  the  way  to  the  hospital.  "Swede" 
Carlson,  the  only  fellow  the  police  caught  at  that  time,  said  that  his 
last  words  were,  "What  will  mother  say?"  The  gang  took  up  a 
collection  for  flowers,  but  the  direct  result  of  the  episode  was  a  desire 
for  revenge.  They  killed  two  negroes  and  "beat  up"  five  more  after 
the  death  of  Shaggy. 

The  standing  of  each  fellow  in  the  gang  was  determined  by  compe- 
tition and  conflict  within  the  group  itself.  Each  member  was  trying 
to  outdo  the  others  in  football  and  everything  else.  There  was  always 
a  struggle  for  the  leadership,  which  usually  went  to  the  best  fighters. 

"Slicker"  Charlie  and  Ellman  were  for  some  reason  or  other  "on  the 
outs,"  and  a  fight  was  arranged  to  see  who  was  better.  The  encounter 
came  off  in  the  park.  Each  fellow  had  his  second,  and  the  time  of 
the  rounds  was  set  just  as  if  it  were  a  regular  prize  fight.  Ellman, 
who  won,  mauled  Charlie  severely,  and  the  latter  fell  into  disgrace,  at 
least  in  his  own  opinion. 

This  feeling  of  his  own  belittlement  caused  Charlie  much  resent- 
ment toward  the  victor  and  led  to  another  fight  in  which  Charlie 
struck  Ellman  with  a  lead  pipe.  The  blood  shot  out  of  a  big  gash  in 
his  head.  After  they  had  taken  him  to  the  emergency  hospital,  a  cop 
came  in  and  wanted  to  know  how  it  had  happened,  but  Ellman  would 
say  nothing  except  that  he  had  fallen  and  his  head  hit  a  rock.  The 
code  of  the  gang  was  that  honor  forbade  squealing.  With  this  incident 
the  feud  came  to  an  end. 

An  example  of  conflict  of  the  play  type,  which  had  a  very  tragic 
outcome,  occurred  one  day  in  the  park.  About  eight  of  the  fellows 
went  to  the  lagoon  and  piled  into  two  tiny  rowboats.  It  was  a  warm 
summer  evening,  and  the  bunch  was  feeling  pretty  good,  so  they  de- 
cided to  have  a  battle.  Splashing  soon  led  to  striking  with  oars.  The 
battle  was  raging  when  one  of  the  boats  went  over.  In  it  was  a  fellow 
called  "Steam,"  who  could  not  swim.  The  others  struck  out  for  the 
shore,  but  Steam  went  down.  As  soon  as  they  discovered  that  he  was 
gone,  they  went  out  and  dived  for  him  until  one  of  them  succeeded 
in  getting  the  body.  The  fire  department  came  and  a  pulmotor  was 
used,  but  to  no  avail.  Before  the  funeral  a  collection  was  taken  up, 
and  an  expensive  floral  piece  was  purchased.  The  gang  turned  to  the 
good  for  one  day,  and  every  member  went  to  the  church.  Steam  was 


302  EGO  IDENTIFICATIONS  IN  GROUP  SITUATIONS 

never  spoken  of  afterward,  for  each  one  of  them  felt  a  little  bit 
responsible  for  his  death. 

Members  of  the  gang  often  engaged  in  shady  exploits  as  individuals 
or  in  pairs.  Ellman  and  "Dago"  were  always  managing  to  make  some 
money  in  one  way  or  another.  At  one  time  Ellman  told  me  of  the 
"booze"  ring,  for  which  he  and  Dago  did  the  delivering.  Where  they 
got  the  booze  I  never  found  out,  but  they  made  $25  or  $30  apiece  for 
a  night's  work  and  gambled  it  away  at  a  place  which  was  a  regular 
Monte  Carlo,  with  tables  for  crap-shooting,  and  caller's  chips  which 
were  purchased  from  the  cashier. 

The  same  pair  were  involved  in  the  robbery  of  a  golf  shelter. 
Owing  to  Ellman's  carelessness,  he  was  followed  and  arrested.  He 
was  convicted  of  petty  larceny  and  put  on  probation,  but  the  police 
could  not  make  him  reveal  the  name  of  his  pal.  By  keeping  mum  he 
saved  Dago  a  lot  of  trouble. 

Another  example  of  loyalty  was  an  incident  which  occurred  when 
the  gang  went  to  Detroit.  Dago  gave  the  money  which  he  was  to 
use  for  carfare  home  to  a  younger  fellow.  Although  it  was  winter, 
he  himself  rode  the  blind.  Since  the  train  took  water  on  the  fly,  he 
was  frozen  to  the  train  when  it  pulled  in. 

The  gang  also  enjoyed  many  quiet  evenings.  It  was  the  rule  for 
the  fellows  to  meet  at  Mike's  on  winter  nights  to  shoot  pool  and  talk. 
In  the  summer  their  hang-out  was  on  the  corner  at  the  entrance  to  the 
park.  There  was  a  tendency  to  stick  together  at  all  times  in  play,  just 
as  in  other  activities.  They  often  went  swimming.  Every  year  they 
played  football,  for  which  they  tried  to  keep  in  training,  and  they 
developed  a  good  team.  The  older  fellows  were  the  leaders  in  their 
athletic  activities. 

One  of  the  exploits  of  the  gang  was  a  migration  from  Chicago  to 
Detroit  when  high  wages  were  being  paid  to  automobile-workers. 
They  rented  a  house  there  and  the  whole  gang  lived  together.  Even 
though  they  were  making  fabulous  wages,  they  did  not  save  a  cent, 
and  finally  came  back  to  Chicago— broke.  It  was  this  Detroit  adven- 
ture that  made  bums  out  of  most  of  them.  They  had  drinking  orgies 
almost  every  night  at  their  house,  and  the  crap  games  took  their 
money. 

The  gang  controlled  its  individual  members,  particularly  when  the 
group  was  together.  As  individuals,  and  in  other  group  relationships 
they  were  not  so  bad,  but  in  the  gang  they  tried  to  act  as  tough  as 
possible.  The  man  who  danced,  who  went  out  with  girls,  or  who 
was  well-mannered  was  ostracized.  Charlie  used  to  act  hard-boiled, 


TOE  STRUCTURAL  PROPERTIES  OF  GROUPS  303 

and  he  even  wore  his  cap  so  that  it  made  him  look  tough.  Ellman, 
who  liked  to  give  the  impression  that  he  was  a  ruffian,  was  going  with 
a  girl  on  the  sly.  When  he  was  with  the  gang  he  was  one  of  the 
meanest  fellows  in  it,  but  when  he  went  out  with  his  girl  he  was  very 
courteous,  quitting  his  loud  talk  and  dropping  his  braggadocian  air. 
In  the  last  few  years  the  gang  has  disintegrated.  There  has  been  a 
tendency  for  its  members  to  be  incorporated  into  the  more  conven- 
tional activities  of  society.  The  majority  of  them  seem  to  have  be- 
come more  settled  in  their  mode  of  life.  Some  have  moved  away. 
Even  the  fellows  who  have  changed,  however,  are  still  pretty  low 
under  the  polished  surface.  Gang  habits  and  influences  still  persist. 
[60,  46-50] 

The  reasons  for  the  disintegration  of  a  gang  give  us  further 
insight  into  the  mechanism  of  group  identification.  We  have  seen 
that  gangs  are  formed  by  boys  as  they  strive  to  relieve  deprivations 
and  secure  for  themselves  a  stable  and  genuine  social  identity.  It  is 
not  a  mere  coincidence,  then,  that  "settling  down"  in  conventional 
society  tends  to  disintegrate  gang  loyalties  or,  at  least,  to  cause 
members  to  drop  out  of  active  gang  participation.  As'  the  former 
associate  of  the  Dirty  Dozen  gang  wrote  in  the  last  paragraph  of 
his  report,  there  is  "a  tendency  for  its  members  to  be  incorporated 
into  the  more  conventional  activities  of  society.  The  majority  of 
them  seem  to  have  become  more  settled  in  their  mode  of  life." 
Desertions  from  the  gang  probably  vary  with  the  degree  of  or- 
ganization and  solidarity  of  the  group. 

According  to  Thrasher,  a  "new  activity  of  settlement,  play- 
ground, or  club"  may  take  away  some  of  the  gang  members. 
Thrasher  cites  marriage,  becoming  a  family  man,  as  one  "of  the 
most  potent  causes  for  the  disintegration  of  the  older  groups."  [36] 
Whyte  summarizes  the  process  concisely: 

To  the  casual  observer  the  corner  gang  seems  to  go  on  for  years 
without  change,  but  actually  changes  are  always  taking  place;  and, 
as  the  men  grow  out  of  their  twenties,  the  gang  itself  tends  to  dis- 
integrate. Some  of  the  members  marry  and  have  children.  Even  if 
they  continue  to  hang  on  the  corner,  their  interests  are  no  longer  con- 
fined to  that  social  area.  With  marriage,  some  move  out  of  Corner- 
ville;  and,  even  when  they  return  to  spend  time  with  the  boys,  they 
are  not  the  active  members  they  once  were.  In  this  period  of  life  the 


304  EGO  IDENTIFICATIONS  IN  GROUP  SITUATIONS 

corner  boy  is  expected  to  "settle  down"  and  find  the  job  that  will 
support  him  and  his  family  in  future  years.  He  becomes  a  different 
fellow,  and  his  gang  either  falls  apart  or  is  included  in  some  larger 
club  organization.  [62,  35] 

Not  all  gang  members  "settle  down"  in  the  orthodox  established 
life  of  society  after  their  adolescent  years  by  getting  a  steady  job, 
marrying,  and  so  on.  Some  go  on  to  participate  in  the  more  grim 
exploits  of  the  adult  gangs.9  In  these  adult  gangs,  group  loyalty 
and  conformity  are  more  exacting,  perhaps,  in  proportion  to  the 
rewards  and  punishments  gang  members  face  as  the  consequence 
of  their  exploits.  As  a  result,  the  structural  properties  and  pres- 
sures of  groups  imposed  on  members  of  adult  gangs  become  more 
rigid.  Deviations  are  punished  more  ruthlessly,  in  many  cases  by 
death.  Landesco  [21]  has  given  close-range  accounts  of  such  adult 
gangs  and  of  their  exacting  structural  properties  in  his  impressive 
study  of  "Organized  Crime  in  Chicago."  There  he  cites  concrete 
instances  of  death  penalties  meted  out  to  deviant  gang  members. 

On  the  basis  of  his  longitudinal  and  intensive  studies  of  the  gang 
world,  Landesco  generalizes  on  the  group  norms  that  emerge  to 
determine  the  attitudes  and  behavior  of  the  gang.  He  comments 
on  how  gangs  at  times  try  to  impose  their  codes  on  the  out-groups 
with  which  they  are  in  contact: 

The  gang  not  only  has  its  own  code  which  governs  the  conduct  of 
its  members,  but  it  even  goes  so  far  as  to  impose  it  upon  outside 
society.  In  recent  years  in  Chicago,  the  public  has  become  familiar 
with  the  bold  practices  of  criminal  gangs  in  terrorizing  witnesses  and 
in  exacting  the  death  penalties  upon  them  and  upon  members  of  the 
gang  who  are  suspected  of  having  given  information  to  the  police. 
An  inside  view  of  the  attitudes  and  codes  of  a  notorious  criminal  gang 
shows  how  a  closely  \nit  group  develops  its  own  standards  and  is 
outraged  and  puzzled  by  the  attempts  to  deal  with  them  according  to 
the  law.  [21, 1055,  italics  ours] 

Landesco  goes  on  to  indicate  that  gang  members  "form  a  group 
dominated  by  the  gangster's  code  of  loyalty,"  that  the  "welfare, 

9  The  study  of  factors  that  determine  whether  a  gang  member  will  "settle  down" 
after  adolescent  years  or  advance  to  membership  in  adult  gangs  should  be  of  great 
significance  psychologically.  We  cannot  diverge  here  to  make  a  special  issue  of  the 
point. 


THE  STRUCTURAL  PROPERTIES  OF  GROUPS  305 

standards,  and  laws  of  organized  society  evoke  no  response  in  their 
hearts  and  minds."  [1057]  He  gives  concrete  illustrations  of 
"gangsters'  mutual  loyalty"  [1053] ;  of  how  "a  man  of  character" 
is  defined  among  the  gang  members  as  prescribed  by  gang  norms 
[1047] ;  and  how,  once  these  norms  are  well  standardized  and  in- 
culcated in  the  youthful  candidates,  "a  stigma  is  soon  attached  to 
legitimate  employment."  [1046] 

Time  and  again,  Landesco  emphasizes  that  it  is  only  by  coming 
into  contact  with  the  pressures  and  punishments  of  society  outside 
of  his  underworld  that  the  gang  member  "gets  his  first  sense  of 
the  necessity  of  justifying  his  behavior."  [1048]  *  For  within  his 
own  group  and  the  underworld  which  constitutes  his  real  psycho- 
logical reference  group,  he  derives  his  relative  status  through  the 
degree  to  which  he  fulfills  and  surpasses  the  prescribed  expecta- 
tions of  his  group.  In  formulating  this  point,  it  seems  to  us  that 
Landesco  has  provided  one  of  the  best  possible  characterizations 
of  the  psychological  properties  of  more  or  less  closed  groups  and 
the  subsequent  conforming  experiences  and  behavior  of  individual 
members. 

The  gangster's  defense  of  his  mode  of  life  arises  only  when  he  comes 
in  contact  with  the  legitimate  outside  world.  Only  then  does  he 
become  conscious  of  a  conflicting  way  of  living.  In  his  own  group, 
on  the  contrary,  he  achieves  status  by  being  a  gangster,  with  gangster 
attitudes,  and  enhances  his  reputation  through  criminal  exploits.  His 
contacts  with  the  police  and  the  courts  and  his  successive  confinements 
in  the  corrective,  reformatory,  and  penal  institutions,  beginning  with 
the  Juvenile  Detention  Home,  then  in  turn  the  Industrial  Training 
School,  the  reformatory  and  the  penitentiary,  gain  him  the  prestige  of 
a  veteran  in  his  group.  His  return  from  the  State  Reformatory  at 
Pontiac  or  from  the  penitentiary  at  Joliet  is  the  occasion  of  sympathy 
and  rejoicing  from  his  gang  brothers.  The  bitterness  engendered 
within  him  by  punishment  and  the  feelings  of  revenge  nurtured  by 
his  mutual  association  with  other  convicts  have  more  deeply  impressed 
upon  him  the  psychology  of  the  criminal  world.  Then,  too,  the 
stigma  which  society  places  upon  him  as  an  ex-convict  identifies  him 
the  more  with  the  underworld.  [21, 1043,  italics  ours] 

In  another  interesting  study,  "The  Woman  and  the  Under- 
world," Landesco  [22]  reports  the  case  of  a  high-grade  Chicago 


306  EGO  IDENTIFICATIONS  IN  GROUP  SITUATIONS 

gangster.  This  study  contains  significant  hints  concerning  the 
psychology  of  group  membership  by  its  inclusion  of  a  concrete 
case  from  real  life  showing  the  dominance  of  established  group 
ties  over  the  promise  of  a  settled  life  with  plenty  of  money  and  an 
attractive  and  refined  woman.  Among  "five  women  who  entered 
the  life  of  Eddie  Jackson,"  the  case  of  the  "Companionate  Woman 
for  Whom  He  Cared"  is  particularly  illuminating  from  this  point 
of  view.  The  companionate  woman  "was  an  American  girl,  born 
in  the  East.  She  was  an  attractive  looking  woman — tall,  about 
five  feet  eight  inches,  weighed  about  130  or  135  pounds,  had 
studied  music  and  had  graduated  from  a  finishing  school.  She 
always  retained  a  maid  in  the  house."  [894] 

Miss 's  father  was  a  manufacturer  in  the  east.    Occasionally  he 

came  to  Chicago  for  a  cure,  which  was  known  as  the  "gold  cure"  and 
was  given  for  $25  a  treatment.  He  took  two  or  three  treatments  a 
week.  [22,  595] 

Miss tried  hard  to  reform  Eddie  Jackson  and  to  settle  down 

with  him  in  the  East.   In  Jackson's  own  words: 

She  wanted  me  to  quit  and  go  to  work.  She  had  plenty  of  money. 
We  had  our  trunks  packed  to  go  to  New  York  and  she  wanted  to 
start  me  in  some  business.  [595] 

She  wanted  to  help  me  start  in  any  business  I  might  choose.  I  had 
some  money.  I  cannot  say  that  I  did  not  have  plenty  of  chances,  but 
I  liked  the  excitement  of  the  racket,  the  politics,  and  the  fixing — the 
successes  and  the  failures.  [22,  896] 

Jackson  finally  made  up  his  mind  to  leave  for  New  York: 

Our  trunks  were  packed  and  I  decided  to  let  her  go  ahead,  and 
follow  five  days  later.  I  never  reached  New  York.  [22,  896] 

The  successes  and  failures  of  his  own  group  had  become  so  well 
ingrained  in  Eddie  Jackson  that  they  weighed  more  heavily  than 
the  settled  life  which  came  so  closely  within  his  reach.  In  Eddie's 
own  words: 

You  see,  there  was  a  good  deal  of  excitement  and  interest,  and  some 
skill  involved  in  the  racket— and  it  was  not  so  easy  to  separate  from 
it  for  the  sake  of  Miss .  [22,  896] 


THE  STRUCTURAL  PROPERTIES  OF  GROUPS  307 

Landesco's  analysis  sheds  light  on  the  dominance  of  group  iden- 
tification over  other  factors  in  this  case: 

Upon  his  [Eddie  Jackson's]  release  from  prison,  he  went  directly  to 
her  apartment.  She  wanted  him  to  leave  Chicago,  to  change  his  ways 
and  friends.  The  prison  experience  had  changed  her  (not  him),  had 
sobered  her;  the  lark  was  over,  the  feast  of  freedom  embittered.  She 
insisted.  He  allowed  her  to  leave.  He  could  not  leave  his  under- 
world.  [22,  901 ,  italics  ours] 

Generality  of  gang  formations.  If  gangs  were  a  phenomena 
occurring  only  in  Chicago  or  a  few  large  cities,  there  could  be  no 
justification  for  generalizing  from  gang  material  about  spontane- 
ous group  formations,  their  psychological  function  for  the  indi- 
vidual, the  rise  of  group  products,  and  their  subsequent  role  in 
regulating  members'  behavior.  However,  sociological  literature 
gives  abundant  evidence  that  spontaneous  group  formations  like 
gangs,  far  from  being  isolated  or  rare  phenomena,  occur  quite 
generally — occur,  in  fact,  wherever  individuals  are  under  the  stress 
of  the  various  factors  already  mentioned  as  important  in  spontane- 
ous group  formation,  as,  for  example,  various  sorts  of  deprivation, 
lack  of  a  stable  social  identity. 

The  almost  universality  of  spontaneous  group  formations  under 
the  stress  of  these  factors  has  been  pointed  out  by  Gist  and  Hal- 
bert.  [18] 

Ganging  is  a  natural  process.  Whether  the  activities  of  a  gang  are 
perverse  or  constructive  depends  upon  the  character  of  the  habitat  of 
the  gang — upon  the  culture  patterns  predominating  in  the  region  and 
upon  the  sequences  of  situations  that  arise  in  the  natural  history  of 
the  gang.  The  gang  is  a  form  of  adjustment  that  boys,  and  even 
girls,  make  whenever  their  family  or  neighborhood  do  not  satisfy 
their  major  wishes  in  a  conventional  way.  [18,  272,  italics  ours] 

In  1925,  Park  [42]  commented  on  the  generality  of  spontaneous 
group  formation,  tracing  much  of  it  back  to  the  play  group,  which 
"under  the  conditions  of  urban  life"  is  assuming  "an  increasing 
importance."  [Ill]  Such  groups  frequently  evolve  into  gangs 
which  "have  exercised  a  considerably  greater  influence  in  forming 
the  character  of  the  boys  who  compose  them  than  has  the  church, 
the  school^  or  any  other  communal  agency  outside  of  the  families 


308  EGO  IDENTIFICATIONS  IN  GROUP  SITUATIONS 

and  homes  in  which  the  members  of  the  gang  are  reared."  [112] 
In  Asbury's  book,  The  Gangs  of  New  Yorl(,  [5]  we  learn  that 
gangs  of  boys  and  young  men  have  existed  since  New  York  City's 
early  days.  He  notes,  for  example,  that  during  and  after  the  Civil 
War,  "as  the  slums  increased  in  extent,  gangsters  of  all  types  and 
ages  multiplied  in  numbers  and  power."  [238]  This  increase  in 
the  number  of  gangsters  was  related  to  the  increasing  number  of 
gangs  often  "composed  of  lads  from  eight  to  twelve  years  of  age." 
[239]  He  also  notes  that  gangs  of  similar  sorts  occur  in  various 
ethnic  and  national  groups,  including  orientals.  From  material 
collected  in  20  American  cities,  Shaw  and  McKay  [53]  show  that 
the  spontaneous  formation  of  gangs  which  engage  in  "delinquent" 
activities  occurs  wherever  the  causes  already  indicated  are  found  to 
exist.  Basing  their  generalizations  on  data  from  such  diverse  cities 
as  Chicago,  Richmond,  Peoria,  Denver,  Boston,  and  Spokane,  the 
authors  conclude: 

"Delinquency — particularly  group  delinquency,  which  constitutes 
a  preponderance  of  all  officially  recorded  offenses  committed  by 
boys  and  young  men — has  its  roots  in  the  dynamic  life  of  the  com- 
munity." [435,  italics  ours]  These  roots  are  "products  of  the  oper- 
ation of  general  processes  more  or  less  common  to  American 
cities."  [415]  Evidence  supporting  the  authors'  statement  that 
such  statistics  may  be  interpreted  in  terms  of  groups  is  given  by 
a  study  of  boys  brought  to  court  in  Cook  County.  This  revealed 
that 

.  .  .  8L8  per  cent  of  these  boys  committed  the  offenses  for  which 
they  were  brought  to  court  as  members  of  groups.  And  when  the 
offenses  were  limited  to  stealing,  it  was  found  that  89  per  cent  of  all 
offenders  were  taken  to  court  as  group  or  gang  members.  [53,  167  f.] 

In  many  additional  cases,  the  influence  of  gang  membership  was 
to  be  seen. 

Studies  of  boy  transients  and  tramps  show  further  how  some 
members  of  the  younger  generation  drift  away  from  established 
conventional  family  life,  from  institutions  such  as  schools  and  the 
church,  and  gravitate  toward  patterns  of  loyalties  and  identifica- 
tions that  have  already  been  informally  created  by  other  individ- 
uals who  had  already  found  the  more  established  ways  of  life  un- 


THE  STRUCTURAL  PROPERTIES  OF  GROUPS  309 

satisfying.  A  youngster  who  "takes  to  the  road"  may  have  his  eyes 
opened  to  the  excitement  and  mobility  of  this  way  of  life  from 
older  and  experienced  members  of  transient  groups.  [2,  256]  [3, 
300]  [4,  756]  [41,  64]  Many  transients  take  to  the  road  during 
adolescence.  This  is  significant  in  view  of  the  factors  that  con- 
tribute to  adolescent  discontent  with  the  established  order  of 
things. 

Since  there  is  always  a  tendency  to  attribute  gang  formations  to 
some  psychological  factors  or  traits  peculiar  to  the  gang  members 
themselves,  we  stress  again  the  fact  that  gang  formations  serve  the 
same  psychological  function  as  any  membership  group  does  for 
its  members— a  school  clique,  a  fraternity  or  sorority,  a  club.  A 
gang  provides  a  social  identification,  a  status.  Thrasher  cites 
many  cases  of  gangs  or  embryonic  gangs  formed  in  the  schools. 
And  we  have  already  seen  how  adolescents  gravitate  towards  each 
other  to  form  more  or  less  confidential  cliques  when  conventional 
settings  do  not  afford  them  social  anchorings  or  identities  in  har- 
mony with  their  developing  longings  and  the  values  of  their  new 
generation.  They  drift  away  to  form  their  own  intimate  social 
cliques,  even  though  some  of  these  may  be  short-lived.  Under 
certain  conditions  these  cliques  may  develop  into  gangs.  Thrasher 
states  that  "a  clique  may  serve  as  the  basis  for  a  gang"  [60,  320] 
or  "in  a  certain  sense  a  well-developed  clique  is  an  embryonic 
gang."  [320]  The  objective  purposes  towards  which  the  group 
(clique  or  gang)  activities  are  directed  may  be  quite  different,  even 
diametrically  opposite.  But  from  the  point  of  view  of  group  for- 
mation, essentially  the  same  psychological  mechanisms  are  at  work 
in  the  various  sorts  of  group  formations.  So  we  must  draw  no 
sharp  and  artificial  distinctions  between  cliques  and  gangs  in 
seeking  the  psychological  principles  involved  in  group  formations. 

The  artificiality  of  drawing  sharp  lines,  in  relation  to  the  basic 
properties  of  groups,  of  conceiving  them  as  closed  and  unchanging 
entities  can  be  shown  in  more  diverse  groups.  Taking  the  ex- 
amples of  a  momentary  group  situation,  caused  by  a  serious  auto- 
mobile accident  on  the  one  hand,  and  the  organized  and  lasting 
group  core  of  a  senate  body  on  the  other  hand,  Sapir  [48]  calls 
attention  to  the  fallacy  of  making  any  arbitrary  demarcations: 


310  EGO  IDENTIFICATIONS  IN  GROUP  SITUATIONS 

There  is  in  reality  no  definite  line  of  division  anywhere  along  the 
gamut  of  group  forms  which  connect  these  extremes.  If  the  auto- 
mobile accident  is  serious  and  one  of  the  members  of  the  crowd  is  a 
doctor,  the  informal  group  may  with  comparatively  little  difficulty 
resolve  itself  into  something  like  a  medical  squad  with  an  implicitly 
elected  leader.  On  the  other  hand,  if  the  government  is  passing 
through  a  great  political  crisis,  if  there  is  little  confidence  in  the  repre- 
sentative character  or  honesty  of  the  senators  or  if  an  enemy  is  be- 
sieging the  capitol  and  likely  at  any  moment  to  substitute  entirely 
new  forms  of  corporate  authority  for  those  legally  recognized  by  the 
citizens  of  the  country,  the  Senate  may  easily  become  an  unimpor- 
tant aggregation  of  individuals  who  suddenly  and  with  unexpected 
poignancy  feel  their  helplessness  as  mere  individuals.  [48,  179] 

Impact  of  society  at  large.  In  our  analysis  of  the  influence  of 
the  group  and  group  products  in  regulating  the  behavior  of  indi- 
vidual members,  it  is  important  to  add  that,  although  once  an 
individual  is  a  member  of  a  group  (gang) — once  he  identifies  him- 
self with  it — the  particular  sort  of  behavior  in  question  (for  ex- 
ample, delinquency)  is  not  due  only  to  contact  with  the  group 
(gang).  In  our  discussion  of  the  essentially  similar  psychological 
functions  served  by  various  spontaneous  group  formations,  and  in 
our  demonstration  of  the  effects  of  group  loyalties  and  identifica- 
tions on  the  behavior  of  individual  members,  we  do  not  even  im- 
ply that  group  formations  of  this  sort  have,  in  the  larger  sense, 
any  existence  independent  of  the  social  milieu  in  which  they  are 
formed.  For  these  groups  are  obviously,  in  turn,  products  of  eco- 
nomic, ethnic,  and  other  major  social  situations  in  the  society  at 
large.  Thus,  Shaw  [49]  and  Zorbaugh  [63]  have  indicated  that 
gangs  engaging  in  delinquent  activities  are  products  of  the  exist- 
ing social  milieu  with  its  social  and  class  structure,  as  Shaw  com- 
ments, of  "processes  more  or  less  common  to  American  cities." 
Anderson  [4],  in  criticizing  his  own  neglect  of  larger  social  condi- 
tions in  studying  transients,  recently  indicated  that  the  factors  giv- 
ing rise  to  transiency  are  inexorably  tied  together  with  sociological, 
technological,  and  economic  conditions  and  changes.  The  very 
factors  which  give  rise  to  spontaneous  groups  are  inevitably  found 
gs  features  of  the  larger  social  system. 


THE  STRUCTURAL  PROPERTIES  OF  GROUPS  311 

By  the  same  token,  the  particular  activities,  standards,  and  the 
like  which  provide  individuals  with  social  standing,  status,  or 
popularity  in  the  larger  society  or  in  a  particular  stratum  or  local- 
ity of  that  larger  society  loom  as  important  in  the  activities  of 
these  more  or  less  well-structured  subgroups.  For  example,  the 
preponderance  of  athletics  in  the  activities  of  boys'  groups  in 
America  today  reflects  the  value  placed  on  such  activities  by  society 
at  large.  The  high  premium  placed  on  such  things  as  feminine 
attractiveness  or  dancing  is  reflected  in  the  activities  of  adolescent 
girls'  cliques.  The  standard  of  living  in  a  society  has  a  major 
influence  on  the  nature  of  a  gang's  exploits.  Stealing  cars  assumes 
wide  proportions  among  delinquent  boys  in  the  United  States. 
The  luxurious  standards  of  an  Al  Capone  and  his  gang  could  be 
found  only  in  a  society  which  makes  possible  "conspicuous  con- 
sumption" of  the  staggering  dimensions  found  among  the  leisure 
class  of  a  Park  Avenue  or  a  Gold  Coast. 

Certain  prohibitions  in  society  may  provide  gangs  with  a  type 
of  activity  that  seeks  definite  economic  rewards.  Zorbaugh  [63] 
mentions,  for  example,  the  importance  of  "bootlegging"  and  "hi- 
jacking" in  the  gang  activities  of  Chicago  at  the  time  of  prohibi- 
tion. [174]  In  some  strata  of  society,  a  high  economic  standing 
may  be  so  keenly  needed  and  highly  valued  that  the  means  by 
which  it  is  acquired  may  be  relatively  unimportant  to  the  members 
of  the  community.  For  example,  the  local  boy  in  the  slums  who 
achieves  a  decent  or  even  luxurious  way  of  life  by  "hi-jacking"  may 
be  regarded  as  a  local  hero,  whereas  outside  of  his  community 
he  is  a  "crook."  In  a  "higher"  level  of  society,  the  means  by 
which  some  financial  magnates  gain  their  fortunes  may  be  over- 
looked by  other  members  of  this  society. 

In  the  last  analysis,  the  major  established  standards  of  success 
or  failure  of  the  gang  or  the  gangster  world  are  derived  from  the 
competitive,  individualistic,  and  financially  hoarding  standards  of 
the  society  at  large.  This  is  clearly  expressed  by  Andrew  A.  Bruce 
in  his  introduction  to  Landesco's  survey  of  "Organized  Crime  in 
Chicago."  [21,  815-821]  Bruce  characterizes  the  general  nature 
of  the  behavior  of  the  gang  or  gangster  world  as  "a  rebellion 
against  organized  society  and  the  laws  under  which  organized 


312  EGO  IDENTIFICATIONS  IN  GROUP  SITUATIONS 

society  has  chosen  to  be  ruled  and  governed."  [5/5]  He  goes  on 
to  say  that  the  major  patterns  of  the  gang  world  are  derived  from 
the  social  system  in  which  it  functions. 

Not  the  least  of  the  disclosures  that  have  been  made  are  those  of 
the  permanence  of  the  reigns  of  the  lords  of  the  underworld  and  the 
introduction  of  the  capitalistic  system  into  their  operations.  [21,  815] 

When  adult  gangs  develop  their  enterprises  to  great  proportions, 
they  may  become  a  real  force  in  the  economic  and  political  life 
of  the  community.  At  the  same  time  they  still  keep  the  structural 
properties  of  the  in-group  intact.  In  such  cases,  rather  close  rela- 
tionships may  be  established  between  business  men,  politicians,  and 
gang  leaders.10 

Similarly,  the  prevalent  intense  competition  between  gang  and 
gang,  clique  and  clique,  club  and  club  reflects  the  intense  social 
and  economic  competition  of  the  larger  culture.  Gang  fights  be- 
tween children  of  various  ethnic,  racial,  or  religious  groups  often 
seem  to  reflect  antagonisms  and  prejudices  of  the  adult  culture. 
In  other  instances,  social,  ethnic,  religious,  and  racial  demarcations 
may  break  down  when  individuals  from  such  groups  are  brought 
closely  together  under  the  powerful  stress  of  deprivations,  similar 
ego-strivings,  identifications,  and  so  on.  These  are  only  some 
examples  that  can  be  cited  of  the  impact  of  the  macrocosm  (larger 
society)  upon  the  microcosm  (gangs,  cliques,  social  movements  of 
various  sorts).  They  cannot  be  neglected  in  any  discussion  of 
spontaneous  group  formation. 

INDIVIDUAL  MEMBERS  AS  INFLUENCED  BY  GROUP  NORMS 
AND  IDENTIFICATIONS 

There  is  substantial  justification  for  the  position  of  certain  psy- 
chologists that  the  social  psychology  of  group  interactions  and  col- 
lective behavior  in  all  its  phases  should  be  worked  out  in  terms  of 
the  experience  and  reactions  of  single  individuals  if  it  is  going  to 
be  psychology  at  all.  In  the  previous  section  we  gave  a  brief  ac- 

10  These  relationships  arc  admirably  indicated  in  Landesco's  survey  [21],  espe- 
cially in  chapter  23  on  "Racketeering,"  chapter  24  on  "The  Gangster  and  the 
Politician,"  and  chapter  25  on  "Funerals  of  Gangsters." 


INDIVIDUAL  MEMBERS  AS  INFLUENCED  BY  GROUP  NORMS          313 

count  of  some  of  the  consequences  of  interaction  in  terms  of  the 
resulting  group  properties  and  their  effects  on  individual  mem- 
bers. In  this  section,  we  shall  analyze  in  more  detail  material 
concerned  with  single  individuals,  and  the  way  these  individuals 
identify  themselves  with  and  participate  in  particular  groups.  As 
we  shall  see,  when  we  approach  group  situations  in  terms  of  the 
single  individual,  we  are  led  to  the  same  conclusions  concerning 
the  social  psychology  of  group  interaction  that  were  reached  when 
we  started  with  the  properties  of  the  group.  Thus  we  can  do 
away  once  more  with  the  individual-group  dichotomy. 

Our  illustrations  will  be  derived  from  "gang"  members,  not, 
however,  because  our  main  concern  is  the  special  type  of  behavior 
exhibited  in  delinquent  groups.  As  we  said  before,  group  be- 
havior shown  in  gangs  merely  affords  psychologists  an  unusually 
good  opportunity  to  study  group  formations  and  differential  group 
behavior  in  a  rather  clear-cut  way.  Such  gang  formations  (under 
given  conditions)  can  be  traced  comparatively  easily.  And  since 
they  are  deviations,  their  major  features  stand  out  in  bold  relief. 

With  the  aid  of  expert  investigators  in  the  various  fields  con- 
cerned, Clifford  Shaw  intensively  studied  boys  from  the  point  of 
view  of  their  socioeconomic  background,  their  psychological 
make-up,  and  their  physical  characteristics.  For  a  good  many 
years  he  followed  closely  the  lives  of  boys  who  participated  as 
members  of  various  gangs  at  different  times.  These  studies  are 
exemplary.  They  give  admirable  accounts  of  the  economic-cul- 
tural setting  in  which  these  boys  gravitate  towards  each  other  to 
form  their  own  group  and  to  participate  in  activities  which  relieve 
their  deprivations.  Shaw  secured  the  "own  stories"  (informal 
autobiographies)  of  some  of  these  boys  and  checked  them  against 
objective  information  he  collected.  In  this  discussion,  we  are  in- 
terested chiefly  in  the  group  (gang)  identifications,  their  effects  in 
forming  appropriate  attitudes  and  in  producing  differential  group 
behavior.  Shaw's  studies  contain  unusually  clarifying  insights 
into  the  problem  of  spontaneous  group  formation  because  of  the 
stress  of  major  deprivations  and  the  lack  of  or  conflict  in  personal 
identity. 

The  "own  stories"  are  especially  important  for  the  psychologist. 
They  show  the  group  identifications,  the  attitudes  formed  in  group 


314  EGO  IDENTIFICATIONS  IN  GROUP  SITUATIONS 

situations,  and  the  behavior  conforming  to  these  attitudes— all  as 
experienced  by  the  participants  themselves.  These  documents  give 
the  "essentially  human"  or  psychological  accounts  of  these  gang 
boys'  lives.  In  the  words  of  Burgess,  "In  the  life-history  is  revealed, 
as  in  no  other  way,  the  inner  life  of  the  person,  his  moral  strug- 
gles, his  successes  and  failures  in  securing  control  of  his  destiny 
in  a  world  too  often  at  variance  with  his  hopes  and  ideals."  [50, 
4]  The  following  "own  story"  of  a  gang  member  furnishes  us, 
for  example,  with  considerable  insight  into  the  dynamics  of  spon- 
taneous group  identifications  and  the  inculcation  of  appropriate 
norms  in  the  participants  of  the  group: 

When  I  started  to  play  in  the  alleys  around  my  home  I  first  heard 
about  a  bunch  of  older  boys  called  the  "Pirates."  My  oldest  brother 
was  in  this  gang  and  so  I  went  around  with  them.  There  were  about 
ten  boys  in  this  gang  and  the  youngest  one  was  eleven  and  the  oldest 
one  was  about  fifteen.  .  .  . 

Tony,  Sollie,  and  my  brother  John  were  the  big  guys  in  the  gang. 
Tony  was  fifteen  and  was  short  and  heavy.  He  was  a  good  fighter 
and  the  young  guys  were  afraid  of  him  because  he  hit  them  and  beat 
them  up.  Sollie  was  a  little  guy  about  twelve  years  of  age.  He 
couldn't  fight,  but  he  was  a  smart  guy  and  told  stories  and  made  plans 
for  the  gang.  He  was  the  brains  of  the  gang.  My  brother  was  fifteen 
and  was  bigger  than  Tony  and  was  a  good  fighter.  He  could  beat 
any  guy  in  the  gang  by  fighting,  so  he  was  a  good  leader  and  every- 
body looked  up  to  him  as  a  big  guy.  I  looked  up  to  him  as  a  big  guy 
and  was  proud  to  be  his  brother.  .  .  . 

When  I  started  to  hang  out  with  the  Pirates  I  first  learned  about 
robbin.  The  guys  would  talk  about  robbin  and  stealing  and  went  out 
on  "jobs"  every  night.  When  I  was  eight  I  started  to  go  out  robbin 
with  my  brother's  gang.  We  first  robbed  junk  from  a  junk  yard  and 
sometimes  from  the  peddlar.  Sometimes  we  robbed  stores.  We  would 
go  to  a  store,  and  while  one  guy  asked  to  buy  something  the  other 
guys  would  rob  anything  like  candy  and  cigarettes  and  then  run.  We 
did  this  every  day.  Sollie  always  made  the  plans  and  Tony  and  John 
would  carry  out  the  plans.  .  .  . 

The  gang  had  a  hangout  in  an  alley  and  we  would  meet  there  every 
night  and  smoke  and  tell  stores  and  plan  for  robbin.  I  was  little  and 
so  I  only  listened.  The  big  guys  talked  about  going  robbin  and  told 


INDIVIDUAL  MEMBERS  AS  INFLUENCED  BY  GROUP  NORMS  315 

stores  about  girls  and  sex  things.  The  guys  always  thought  about 
robbin  and  bummin  from  school  and  sometimes  from  home.  .  .  . 

Besides  robbin,  the  gang  went  bummin  downtown  and  to  ball  parks 
and  swimming.  On  these  trips  we  always  robbed  everything  we 
could  get.  .  .  . 

When  I  was  ten  the  gang  started  to  robbin  stores  and  homes.  We 
would  jimmy  the  door  or  window  and  rob  the  place.  I  always  stayed 
outside  and  gave  jiggers.  The  big  guys  went  in  and  raided  the  place. 
They  showed  me  how  to  pick  locks,  jimmy  doors,  cut  glass,  and  use 
skeleton  keys  and  everything  to  get  into  stores  and  houses.  Every  guy 
had  to  keep  everything  a  secret  and  not  tell  anybody  or  he  would  be 
beat  up  and  razzed.  The  police  were  enemies  and  not  to  be  trusted. 
When  we  would  get  caught  by  the  police  we  had  to  keep  mum  and 
not  tell  a  word  even  in  the  third  degree. 

/  looked  up  to  my  brother  and  the  other  big  guys  because  of  their 
courage  and  nerve  and  the  way  they  could  rob.  They  would  tell  me 
never  to  say  a  word  to  anybody  about  our  robbin.  My  mother  didn't 
even  \now  it.  Some  kids  couldn't  be  in  the  gang  because  they  would 
tell  everything  and  some  didn't  have  the  nerve  to  go  robbin.  The  guys 
with  a  record  were  looked  up  to  and  admired  by  the  young  guys.  A 
stool-pigeon  was  looked  down  on  and  razzed  and  could  not  stay  in 
the  gang.  .  .  . 

The  guys  stucJ{  together  and  helped  each  other  out  of  trouble.  They 
were  real  good  pals  and  would  stick  up  for  each  other.  They  were 
always  planning  new  crimes  and  new  ways  to  get  by  without  being 
caught.  Everyone  hated  the  police  and  looked  upon  them  as  enemies. 
Anybody  who  was  friendly  to  the  police  was  not  trusted.  The  plans 
for  stealing  were  always  secret  and  anybody  who  talked  about  them 
to  fellows  outside  of  the  gang  or  to  the  police  was  not  trusted  and 
became  an  enemy  of  the  Pirates.  .  .  .  [50,  10f.f  italics  ours] 

Clifford  Shaw  published  these  intensive  longitudinal  studies  and 
"own  stories"  in  The  ]ac\  Roller  (1930)  [50],  The  Natural  His- 
tory of  a  Delinquent  Career  (1931)  [51],  and  Brothers  in  Crime 
(1938)  [52].  His  work  on  the  "Jack  Roller"  (Stanley)  is  an  inten- 
sive account  of  a  boy  who  participated  in  delinquent  activities  and, 
at  last,  settled  down  to  conventional  society  for  good.  Checking 
Stanley's  own  story  against  the  objective  data  at  his  command  and 
his  contacts  with  Stanley  for  five  years,  Shaw  states  that  "the  sin- 
cerity of  the  story  cannot  be  questioned"  and  that  "the  story  re- 
veals his  fundamental  attitudes  and  typical  reactions  to  the  various 


316  EGO  IDENTIFICATIONS  IN  GROUP  SITUATIONS 

situations  in  which  he  has  lived."  [50,  47]  As  we  shall  see,  Stan- 
ley's account  tells  us  a  great  deal  about  spontaneous  group  forma- 
tions and  identifications  when  there  is  deprivation  and  lack  of 
stable  social  links. 

Stanley  was  first  studied  by  Dr.  William  Healy  when  he  was 
seven  years  and  ten  months  old.  He  was  found  to  be  a  little  above 
the  average  in  intelligence  and  of  about  normal  physical  condition. 
He  seemed 

...  on  the  whole  to  be  a  very  nice  boy.  During  the  examination 
he  talked  freely.  [50, 198] 

He  had  a  "winning  smile."  [198] 

The  psychological  tests  at  the  age  of  eighteen  showed  an  intelligence 
quotient  of  1.06.  [50,  799] 

Shaw  gives  a  rather  detailed  account  of  the  social  and  economic 
areas  of  Chicago  in  which  Stanley's  family  lived  at  different  pe- 
riods. They  were  slum  areas  with  a  relatively  high  degree  of 
poverty  and  with  conflicting  immigrant  and  American  values. 
Concerning  one  of  the  areas  in  which  Stanley's  family  lived,  Shaw 
states: 

In  the  present  Polish  neighborhood  back  of  the  yards,  with  a  popu- 
lation in  1920  of  52.1  per  cent  foreign-born,  there  is  a  definite  break 
between  the  foreign-born  parents  and  their  native-born  children.  [50, 

*\ 

Stanley,  the  American  born  son  of  a  Polish  immigrant  worker,  was 
four  years  old  when  his  mother  died.  A  few  months  later  his 
father  married  a  woman  who  brought  with  her  "seven  children 
from  two  previous  marriages." 

The  stepmother  favored  her  own  children  and  discriminated  against 
Stanley  and  his  brother  and  sister.  [50,  42] 

Throughout  his  story,  Stanley  complains  bitterly  about  the  treat- 
ment he  received  from  her.  With  over  ten  children  in  the  family, 
they  went  hungry  most  of  the  time. 

Under  the  stress  of  hunger  and  other  conditions,  Stanley  started 
his  real  life  activities  in  petty  stealing,  led  by  his  step-brother  and 
other  boys  a  few  years  older.  He  kept  himself  away  from  the  re- 


INDIVIDUAL  MEMBERS  AS  INFLUENCED  BY  GROUP  NORMS  317 

strained  and  miserable  home  situation.  He  entered  school  at  the 
age  of  six  but  played  hooky  a  great  deal. 

Despite  his  frequent  truancies,  he  was  graduated  at  the  age  of 
thirteen  and  a  half,  while  in  the  St.  Charles  School  for  Boys.  [50,  32] 

His  genuine  psychological  identifications  and  attitudes  seem  to 
have  been  little  affected  by  the  school,  although  it  undoubtedly 
tried  to  mold  him  in  the  usual  routine  way.  His  real  identification 
was  with  a  gang  he  joined  at  the  age  of  seven.  This  particular 
gang  "consisted  of  about  twelve  members,  who  ranged  in  age  from 
six  to  seventeen  years."  [44]  Membership  in  this  gang  and,  later, 
in  other  gangs  gave  Stanley  a  personal  status  and  identity  he  did 
not  find  in  his  family  or  school.  It  was  only  as  a  member  of  the 
gang  that  he  was  not  "looked  down"  upon. 

His  delinquent  activities  began  while  he  was  in  company  with 
this  group.  As  a  member  of  this  group  and  other  gangs  he  ad- 
vanced progressively  from  "truancy  and  petty  stealing  at  the  early 
age  of  six  years,  to  the  more  serious  delinquency  of  'jack-rolling' 
and  burglary,  in  the  adolescent  period."  [25]  Arrests  and  commit- 
ments followed  each  other,  so  that  Stanley  spent  almost  half  his 
youthful  life  in  correctional  institutions. 

Our  chief  concern  in  Stanley's  story  is  the  effect  of  group  mem- 
bership on  his  identity  and  behavior.  From  this  point  of  view,  one 
of  his  experiences  can  serve  as  a  starting  point  for  the  further 
examination  of  his  story: 

One  day  my  partner  didn't  show  up,  and  right  then  and  there  I 
lost  all  my  nerve.  I  needed  someone  with  me  to  steal.  I  was  too 
cowardly  to  steal  alone.  A  companion  made  me  brave  and  gave  me 
a  sense  of  security.  I  couldn't  to  save  my  soul  steal  a  dime  alone. 
[50,  86,  italics  ours] 

Without  implying  in  the  least  that  all  delinquent  behavior  is  a 
function  of  group  behavior,  this  experience  of  Stanley's  is  signifi- 
cant. It  gives  a  typical  illustration  of  the  psychological  qualities 
of  a  group  situation,  irrespective  of  the  direction  group  activities 
take.  It  is  not  unusual,  for  example,  to  hear  a  war  veteran  cite 
the  case  of  some  fellow  who  surprised  his  buddies  by  unexpected 
bravery— for  him— during  some  active  combat. 


318  EGO  IDENTIFICATIONS  IN  GROUP  SITUATIONS 

".  .  .  the  life  in  the  streets  and  alleys  became  fascinating  and 
enticing"  to  Stanley  very  soon  after  he  reached  school  age.  In 
the  streets  he  found  two  close  companions,  Tony  and  William, 
whom  he  regarded  as  heroes.  He  soon  developed  so  he  fit  into 
their  pattern,  as  any  growing  boy  might  fit  into  the  pattern  of  his 
heroes,  whatever  their  characteristics  might  be. 

To  my  child-seeing  eyes,  I  visioned  Tony  as  a  great  leader  in  the 
neighborhood,  and  he  directed  his  gang  around  with  much  bravado. 
He  and  William  were  always  stealing  and  talking  about  stealing  and 
I  fell  in  with  them  as  soon  as  I  began  to  play  around  in  the  neighbor- 
hood. [50,  51] 

Stanley  identified  himself  so  closely  with  the  gang  he  happened 
to  belong  to  at  the  time,  was  so  anchored  in  its  social  setting,  that 
he  felt  personally  secure  only  in  its  atmosphere.  We  see  his  loss 
of  ego  bearings  when  he  was  outside  this  atmosphere. 

From  the  time  I  used  to  go  to  the  markets  and  to  West  Madison 
Street  with  the  old  gang  I  had  been  attracted  to  throngs  of  people,  not 
the  Loop  throngs,  but  the  West  Madison  and  South  State  Street 
throngs.  /  could  not  explain  this  irresistible  interest,  even  if  I  wanted 
to.  Perhaps  it  was  the  telepathy  that  is  from  one  derelict  to  another. 
I  do  know  full  well  that  this  human  wreckage  was  always  full  of  in- 
terest and  mystery  to  my  dreamy  mind.  Men  of  all  nationalities  and 
races,  from  the  four  corners  of  the  earth,  were  there  and  brushed 
shoulders  with  the  crooks  and  gunmen  of  the  underworld.  They 
were  all  attracted  there,  as  I  was,  by  the  cheap  movies,  flophouses, 
cheap  hashhouses,  and,  most  of  all,  by  the  human  derelicts  that  make 
West  Madison  Street  what  it  is.  When  blue  and  bro\en-up  I  would 
always  find  an  old  pal  there  to  tell  my  troubles  to  and  receive  the  sym- 
pathy that  comes  through  mutual  understanding.  All  the  old  bums 
and  human  wrecks  were  my  family.  We  all  ate  at  the  same  table  and 
enjoyed  ourselves  at  the  same  theaters.  In  fact,  we  consisted  of  a 
brotherhood  whose  object  was  mutual  pity  and  sympathy.  The 
brotherhood  was  made  up  of  ordinary  "bos,"  pickpockets,  pan-handlers, 
petty  thieves,  "jack-rollers,"  and  the  other  wrecks  that  compose  the 
underworld.  Here  was  my  favorite  haunt,  because  my  friends  made 
their  rendezvous  there.  It  seemed  to  me  that  here  the  lights  gleamed 
brighter,  the  lures  were  stronger,  and  that  there  were  more  bums  to 
hide  me  from  the  stares  of  snobbish  people.  [50,  79  /.,  italics  ours] 


INDIVIDUAL  MEMBERS  AS  INFLUENCED  BY  GROUP  NORMS  319 

But  not  any  kind  of  a  life  that  appears  to  be  settled  is  sufficient 
to  make  one  feel  psychologically  settled,  even  though  it  may  afford 
food,  shelter,  and  luxuries.  When  Stanley  was  an  adolescent  boy, 
he  was  taken  into  the  home  of  the  vice-president  of  a  company 
where  he  worked  for  a  time.  The  vice-president  who  "was  mar- 
ried but  had  no  children,"  even  planned  to  adopt  him.  But  Stan- 
ley felt  he  did  not  belong  there.  Something  was  missing.  He 
longed  for  his  pals,  in  whose  company  he  felt  himself  at  ease  and 
a  person  in  his  own  right. 

The  surroundings  in  my  new  home  and  neighborhood  took  my 
breath  away.  My  first  day  at  the  foster-home  was  like  a  sweet  dream. 
The  new  luxury  seemed  to  dazzle  and  blind  me.  My  new  father 
rode  with  me  to  work  every  morning  and  home  in  the  evening.  We 
had  nice  lunches  together  at  noon.  He  talked  nice  to  me,  gave  me 
spending  money  and  good  clothes,  but  I  missed  my  old  pals  and  the 
gay  life  we  had  lived.  Here  I  did  not  have  any  boy  chums,  but  had  to 
spend  my  time  playing  the  victrola.  My  foster-parents  didn't  have 
much  life,  but  spent  their  time  reading  and  playing  a  tame  game  of 
cards.  They  had  lots  of  company  of  snobbish  people,  and  they  looked 
down  on  me.  Even  if  they  were  nice,  it  was  because  of  pity  and 
charity.  There  was  something  missing.  Eating  at  the  table  I  was 
ill  at  ease.  I  couldn't  do  the  things  just  right,  and  my  foster-mother 
looked  at  my  blunders  through  the  corner  of  her  eye.  I  compared 
everything  with  my  sister's  common  fare  and  poor  surroundings,  and 
•finally  longed  to  go  bac\  to  my  friends  and  pals.  Back  home  I  wasn't 
dressed  up  all  the  time,  and  could  play  and  romp  and  gamble  and 
swear.  But  here  I  was  not  free  to  move  and  talk  as  I  was  in  the 
habit  of  doing  before.  Everything  was  different — strange  and  stiff. 
/  felt  out  of  place— z  city  waif  dependent  upon  charity.  I  had  been  in 
jail  half  a  lifetime,  but  now  I  was  suddenly  placed  in  luxury  after 
living  in  a  dirty  hovel.  My  adventurous  spirit  rebelled  against  this 
dry  life  and  it  soon  won  out.  [50,  87  f.,  italics  ours] 

After  leaving  this  home,  Stanley  was  again  in  the  gang  atmos- 
phere, in  his  own  element. 

The  lures  and  the  irresistible  call  drew  me  on  like  a  magnet.  I  was 
always  helpless  before  them.  I  was  like  a  canoe  on  a  storm-swept  sea, 
buffeted  here  and  there,  helpless  and  frail.  /  had  about  as  much 
chance  of  controlling  my  desires  to  drift  with  the  current  of  the  under- 


320  EGO  EDENTinCATIONS  IN  GROUP  SITUATIONS 

world  as  the  canoe  had  of  braving  the  storm.  But  here  I  mingled 
with  bums  and  derelicts  like  myself,  and  people  did  not  stare  at  my 
rags  and  misery.  Here  I  felt  at  home,  for  "misery  loves  company." 
So  I  drifted  on  with  the  rest  of  the  human  driftwood — carried  on  by 
»the  current  of  West  Madison  Street's  exclusive  "Four  Hundred*5  or 
more.  [50,  93,  italics  ours] 

In  this  social  atmosphere,  Stanley  enjoyed  the  prestige  acquired 
from  his  relative  status  in  his  new  gang. 

There  v/ere  four  of  us  who  hung  around  together.  The  other  three 
had  been  in  St.  Charles  School  for  Boys  while  I  was  there,  and  that 
strengthened  our  faith  in  each  other.  /  was  loo\ed  up  to  as  the  hero 
of  the  quartet  because  I  had  done  fifty-six  months  in  St.  Charles,  more 
than  all  the  others  put  together.  They  naturally  thought  I  was  one 
who  had  a  vast  experience  and  was  regarded  as  one  might  regard  the 
big  social  hit  of  society.  [50,  96,  italics  ours] 

Once  groups  or  gangs  are  formed,  reciprocal  loyalties  develop 
among  the  members.  Norms  arise  more  or  less  informally  but 
nevertheless  become  binding  within  the  group  and  are  inculcated 
as  personal  values  in  the  ego  structure  of  the  members.  Stanley 
speaks  of  these  reciprocal  loyalties: 

My  fellow-workers  [in  the  gang]  were  fast  guys  and  good  pals, 
We  were  like  brothers  and  would  stick  by  each  other  through  thick 
and  thin.  We  cheered  each  other  in  our  troubles  and  loaned  each 
other  dough.  A  mutual  understanding  developed,  and  nothing  could 
break  our  confidence  in  each  other.  [50,  96] 

In  a  similar  way,  an  atmosphere  of  solidarity  is  established  in 
jail.  Cliques  or  gangs  are  formed  spontaneously  on  the  basis  of 
common  interest  and  in  the  face  of  the  common  opposition  to 
authority. 

In  the  tailor  shop  [of  the  penal  institution  to  which  he  was  com- 
mitted on  this  particular  occasion]  were  two  pals  that  I  had  known 
in  Pontiac.  So  we  became  friends  immediately  and  helped  each  other 
out  by  exchanging  reading  materials,  tobacco,  and  by  giving  each 
other  warnings  and  inside  tips  about  how  to  get  by.  //  was  a  little 
mutual  aid  society,  which  is  very  necessary  in  prison.  The  prisoners 
have  to  band  together  for  their  own  protection,  [50, 155,  italics  ours] 


INDIVIDUAL  MEMBERS  AS  INFLUENCED  BY  GROUP  NORMS          321 

Once  incorporated,  the  norms  of  this  more  or  less  isolated  prison 
group  may  also  be  experienced  as  intensely  felt  personal  values  in 
much  the  same  way  that  the  norms  of  any  social  group  are  ex- 
perienced. Stanley's  account  illustrates  this  point: 

To  squawk  on  a  fellow-prisoner  is  an  unpardonable  sin  and  only 
the  lowest  characters  will  squawk.  But  there  were  boys  who  would 
squawk  and  they  would  usually  become  boy  officers,  so  we  did  not 
trust  them  but  harbored  hatred  toward  them.  They  were  not  fit  to  be 
associated  with  decent  boys.  [50,  67,  italics  ours] 

We  see  from  Stanley's  own  account  how  he  made  the  norms  of 
the  isolated  prison  situation  a  part  of  him: 

I  fell  in  the  web  without  any  experience,  but  soon  got  onto  the 
ropes.  My  feeling  was  for  the  code  and  against  the  officials.  Don't 
trust  anybody  except  tried  pals  who  won't  squawk.  [50,  67] 

Group  norms  may  become  so  well  incorporated  as  personal  ego- 
attitudes  that  individual  group  members  will  observe  them  at  the 
cost  of  personal  punishment  and  hardship.  This  fact  is  concretely 
illustrated  in  Stanley's  story: 

One  Saturday  afternoon  all  of  us  were  playing  on  the  drill  field, 
and  the  first  lieutenant  (a  boy)  asked  me  to  go  to  the  cottage  and 
bring  his  harmonica.  While  I  was  in  the  cottage  I  saw  two  other  boys 
who  had  sneaked  into  the  cottage,  and  they  were  stealing  something. 
I  took  the  harmonica  to  the  lieutenant  and  ten  minutes  later  I  was 
accused  of  stealing  some  cigars  from  the  basement.  I  denied  the 
charge,  and  could  have  cleared  myself  by  telling  on  the  two  boys,  but 
I  wouldn't  squawk  and  break  the  code;  consequently,  I  was  given  a 
good  beating  and  forced  to  do  "haunches"  another  hour.  Many  times 
did  I  suffer  because  I  wouldn't  squawk,  but  I'd  die  before  I'd  turn  on 
a  fellow  prisoner.  [50,  69] 

And  just  as  good  members  of  any  organized  group  uphold  the 
values  or  norms  of  the  group,  expect  the  same  of  other  members, 
and  impose  various  correctives  and  punishments  on  those  who  de- 
viate, so  the  good  members  of  gangs  become  conscious  of  their 
own  norms  and  react  violently  against  deviants  and  nonconform- 
ists. Stanley  believed 


322  EGO  IDENTIFICATIONS  IN  GROUP  SITUATIONS 

.  .  .  that  any  game  should  be  played  according  to  the  rules  of  the 
game.  Violators  of  rules  should  be  punished.  Crime  is  a  game,  and 
therefore  as  a  rat  violates  the  rules  or  code  by  informing  the  "dicks" 
and  the  "screws,"  he  should  be  punished  when  caught,  just  like  other 
criminals  are  punished.  I  think  everyone  will  agree  with  me  in  my 
feelings  about  these  low  rats.  All  prisoners  who  are  worthy  of  the 
name  will  agree  with  me.  [50, 112] 

We  saw  in  the  last  section  that  when  gang  members  "settled 
down"  in  conventional  society,  they  tended  to  pull  away  from  the 
gang.  This  happened  to  Stanley.  Through  Clifford  Shaw's  in- 
sightful analysis  of  the  case  and  his  effective  help  in  placing  Stan- 
ley in  situations  which  contributed  to  his  settling  down  without 
making  him  feel  out  of  place,  Stanley  eventually  conformed  to 
the  norms  of  conventional  society.  When  about  18,  Stanley  was 
released  from  the  House  of  Correction,  identifying  "himself  with 
the  adult  criminal  group."  [164]  Shaw  gave  him  new  clothes 
and  placed  him  in  a  congenial  home,  where,  as  Stanley  reported, 
"They  seemed  to  accept  me  and  not  look  down  on  me."  After 
several  trials  in  jobs,  he  became  a  salesman  and  showed  consider- 
able ability  in  this  work,  in  which  there  was  not  much  chance  to 
be  reminded  constantly  of  his  past  record.  He  married  a  girl  who 
told  him  that  she  did  not  care  what  he  had  done,  but  was  inter- 
ested in  what  he  was  going  to  be.  He  enjoyed  the  company  of 
her  other  young  friends.  Encouraged  by  Shaw,  he  enrolled  in 
evening  classes  to  complete  his  high  school  education.  But 
throughout  all  this  period,  his  old  pals  in  the  slum  area  still  exerted 
influence  over  him.  In  Shaw's  words: 

The  influence  of  these  earlier  relationships  did  not  begin  to  diminish 
until  other  interests  and  relationships  were  established  in  his  new 
situation.  We  may  assume  that  the  gradual  changes  which  are  taking 
place  in  Stanley's  conduct  throughout  this  chapter  reflect  changes  in 
his  group  relationships.  [50, 175] 

Since  thousands  of  young  men,  who  may  identify  themselves  with 
the  adult  of  the  criminal  group  at  the  end  of  their  release  from 
prison,  do  not  have  the  expert  guidance  of  an  understanding  per- 
son, it  is  likely  that  many  of  them  will  follow  the  lead  of  their 
more  mature  and  grim  gang  identifications  with  all  the  conse- 


INDIVIDUAL  MEMBERS  AS  INFLUENCED  BY  GROUP  NORMS          323 

quences  until  they  can  be  absorbed  in  work  and  in  interpersonal 
relationships  with  society  at  large  through  the  transformation  of 
ego  identifications.11 

Another  case  presented  in  The  Natural  History  of  a  Delinquent 
Career  [51]  is  particularly  interesting,  because  it  traces  the  effects 
of  gang  influence  on  the  identifications,  attitudes,  and  activities  of 
a  boy,  Sidney,  who  differed  in  some  important  respects  from  Stan- 
ley, the  "jack  roller."  The  results  of  Sidney's  intelligence  test  at 
the  age  of  15  indicated  "excellent  mental  development"  and  placed 
"him  in  the  group  having  superior  intelligence."  His  intelligence 
quotient  was  reported  as  126. 

The  physical  and  neurological  examinations  were  negative.    There 
was  no  indication  of  mental  pathology.  [51,  267] 

The  community  in  which  Sidney  grew  up  was  one  of  the  worst 
slum  areas  in  the  city.  He  was  the  youngest  of  two  children  in 
a  poor,  often  "destitute"  Jewish  family.  Shortly  after  Sidney  was 
born, 

Because  of  the  father's  repeated  desertions,  the  mother  was  forced 
to  seek  employment  outside  of  home.  [51,  230] 

His  brother,  Abe,  seven  years  older,  "was  in  most  respects  a 
model  person"  of  "average  intelligence." 

The  mother  exercised  much  closer  supervision  of  Abe  than  it  was 
possible  for  her  to  exercise  in  the  case  of  Sidney.  [51,  234] 

Abe  did  not  become  associated  with  neighborhood  gangs. 

Sidney's  first  social  experiences  outside  of  the  home  were  with 
a  neighborhood  play  group  made  up  of  children  four  or  five  years 
older  than  himself.  As  we  shall  see,  Sidney  rather  consistently 
associated  and  managed  to  keep  up  with  groups  of  boys  older  than 
himself.  Possibly  he  was  able  to  do  so  partly  because  he  was  un- 
usually intelligent.  His  delinquent  activities  (petty  stealing)  be- 
gan at  about  the  age  of  seven  after  his  entrance  into  this  group. 
Sidney  wrote  of  these  early  stealing  experiences: 

11  For  an  account  of  the  development  of  identifications  with  the  criminal  world, 
as  well  as  for  material  relevant  to  gang  effects  and  the  impact  of  the  community 
and  larger  society  on  youthful  gangs,  see  [59,  51-81] . 


324  EGO  IDENTIFICATIONS  IN  GROUP  SITUATIONS 

Never  a  thought  occurred  to  me  as  to  whether  it  was  right  or  wrong, 
it  was  merely  an  interesting  game.  The  apple  or  orange  didn't  ma\e 
as  much  difference  as  the  getting  of  them.  It  was  the  taking  them 
that  I  enjoyed.  [55] 

One  night  I  remember  I  rounded  up  a  few  of  my  acquaintances 
and  invited  them  down  to  one  of  these  fruit  stores  and  showed  them 
how  I  could  get  away  with  things.  I  stole  about  a  dozen  large  beets. 
At  another  time  I  stole  for  the  same  bunch  some  tomatoes  or  some- 
thing else  to  eat.  Everyone  had  a  fine  time  and  cheered  me  on  to 
further  efforts.  I  felt  fine  at  achieving  such  success  in  their  eyes.  But 
a  few  of  them  tattled  to  my  brother  and  I  never  surrounded  myself 
with  an  audience  after  that.  I  don't  mean  that  afterwards  I  stole  by 
myself.  The  fact  is  that  I  never  stole  when  I  was  by  myself.  The 
kick  came  when  there  was  someone  with  me  and  the  fun  could  be 
mutual.  It  was  a  merry,  exciting  pastime  that  interested  me  to  the 
exclusion  of  all  others.  [51,  59  /.,  italics  ours] 

Shaw  verifies  the  fact  that  Sidney  stole  almost  exclusively  in  the 
company  of  others  and  adds  that  "more  than  90  per  cent  of  the 
stealing  offenses  in  cases  brought  before  the  Cook  County  court 
are  committed  by  groups  of  two  or  more  boys."  [59] 

During  this  period,  Sidney  began  to  play  truant  from  school, 
not  so  much  because  he  disliked  school  as  because  it  seemed  dull 
compared  to  the  thrilling  activities  of  his  gang. 

Despite  his  repeated  truancy,  his  school  report  shows  a  record  of 
good  scholarship.  [51,  231] 

Because  Sidney  was  much  younger  and  smaller  than  .the  other 
gang  members,  he  seldom  got  a  fair  cut  of  the  booty.  They  would 
encourage  him  to  pick  a  fight  with  an  older  boy  and  then  enjoy 
seeing  him  "take"  the  bigger  boy.  But  Sidney  "didn't  like  this  as 
I  wanted  to  be  one  of  them  and  not  the  object  of  their  amuse- 
ment"  [73,  italics  ours] 

After  several  experiences  in  the  Parental  School  for  delinquents 
and  truants,  Sidney  moved  with  his  family  to  a  new  neighborhood. 
His  mother  bought  him  his  first  new  suit.  For  a  year,  he  skated, 
played  baseball,  and  so  on,  with  the  boys  in  this  school  and  "was 
a  model  pupil."  But  the  following  winter,  he  decided  to  go  to 
work,  was  picked  up  by  the  truant  officer  who  akeady  knew  him 


INDIVIDUAL  MEMBERS  AS  INFLUENCED  BY  GROUP  NORMS          325 

well,  and  was  sent  oft  to  the  Parental  School  again.   This  was  his 
fourth  term  in  this  detention  home. 

On  his  release,  Sidney  started  back  to  school  again.  The  "nice" 
boys  and  girls  were  not  intimate  with  him.  As  he  wrote: 

I  was  very  unhappy  and  couldn't  find  anything  to  do  that  would 
interest  me  continuously.  I  knew  that  if  I  could  get  in  with  a  gang 
I  would  find  plenty  of  excitement  and  thrills,  but  I  didn't  know  of 
any  gang.  It  wasn't  my  first  choice,  but  it  was  a  good  substitute  and 
I  knew  it  was  interesting  because  I  had  belonged  to  a  gang  when  I 
had  been  younger.  Excitertient  seemed  to  be  my  natural  impulse  and 
I  greatly  desired  companionship— some  one  with  similar  desires  as 
mine.  I  wanted  lots  of  fun.  /  fyiew  how  to  get  it  in  the  legitimate 
way,  but  couldn't.  So  I  wanted  to  go  with  some  gang  who  broke  the 
laws  because  I  knew  I  could  soon  gain  their  attitudes  because  it 
would  be  naturally  somewhat  similar  to  the  attitude  of  my  old  gang. 
[51, 113,  italics  ours] 

Finally,  Sidney  "dropped  over  to  the  west  side  to  see  what 
became  of  the  old  gang."  [117]  Afterward,  he  met  several  boys 
acquainted  with  older  criminals  at  the  Burns  Athletic  Club,  whose 
membership,  according  to  Shaw,  included  "some  of  Chicago's 
most  notorious  criminal  characters."  [131]  As  Shaw  notes: 

Prior  to  Sidney's  contact  with  the  Burns  Athletic  Club  his  delin- 
quencies had  been  limited  very  largely  to  pilfering,  burglary,  and 
shoplifting.  ...  It  was  immediately  following  his  association  with 
this  group  and  while  in  the  company  of  some  of  its  younger  members 
that  Sidney's  first  experience  in  the  larceny  of  automobiles  and  holdup 
with  a  gun  took  place.  [51,  232] 

Because  he  had  no  "vocational  and  leisure-time  interests"  and  was 
not  "incorporated"  into  any  more  or  less  permanent  group,  he 
easily  accepted  "the  adult  criminal  pattern  of  this  group." 
These  criminals  were  heroes  to  Sidney. 

The  three  fellows  that  I  went  with  lived  in  the  neighborhood  all 
their  lives  and  they  told  me  the  history  of  many  of  these  racketeers. 
They  knew  them  all  personally  and  of  course  I  got  to  know  them 
personally.  Just  knowing  them  made  us  feel  like  big  shots  and  I 
longed  to  be  able  to  carry  a  big  forty-five  automatic  like  they  carried 
on  their  hip.  My  life  had  been  lawless  and  I  felt  that  some  day  to 


326  EGO  IDENTIFICATIONS  IN  GROUP  SITUATIONS 

become  a  big  gangster  would  be  a  fine  thing  for  me.   I  wanted  to  be 
one  tough  guy.  [51, 136] 

It  is  evident  that  this  group  of  criminals  became  the  reference 
group  with  which  Sidney  and  his  friends  identified  themselves. 

When  he  was  16  years  old,  he  was  sentenced  to  20  years  in 
prison  for  rape  at  the  point  of  a  gun.  From  Sidney's  "own  story" 
we  gather  that,  until  he  knew  that  the  police  had  been  informed 
of  this  act,  he  thought  of  it  as  an  exciting  experience,  as  some- 
thing to  brag  about  at  the  Burns  Athletic  Club.  Shaw  points  out 
that  this  offense  "was  an  integral  part  of  his  whole  criminal- 
behavior  pattern."  [252] 

It  is  perhaps  important  to  observe,  also,  that  prior  to  the  first  rape 
episode  in  which  this  gang  was  involved  a  number  of  similar  cases 
had  occurred  in  the  Chicago  district.  These  cases  were  known  to 
Sidney  and  his  companions  and  had  been  the  subject  of  discussion 
among  them.  ...  It  may  be  assumed  that  his  attitudes  toward  women 
and  sex  behavior  were  defined  through  his  experience  with  prosti- 
tutes and  in  the  course  of  his  conversations  with  other  delinquents. 
He  was  never  incorporated  into  a  conventional  group  through  which 
he  might  assimilate  the  conventional  attitudes  and  moral  values  of 
society.  For  the  most  part,  his  contacts  with  conventional  groups 
were  not  only  casual  and  infrequent  but  essentially  formal  and  external 
in  character.  [51,  233,  italics  ours] 

This  does  not  mean,  of  course,  that  Sidney  was  ignorant  of  the 
norms  of  larger  society  concerning  sex.  But  it  does  indicate  that 
the  norms  of  larger  society  were  not  his.  Identification  with  the 
adult  criminal  group  was  for  him  a  more  potent  determinant  of 
behavior  than  mere  knowledge  of  the  norms  of  society.  Without 
attempting  to  analyze  all  the  important  factors  in  this  case,  we  can 
report  Shaw's  own  conclusion  after  his  study  of  the  records  of  each 
member  of  the  major  gangs  Sidney  identified  himself  with: 

The  foregoing  records  suggest  that  Sidney's  delinquencies  occurred 
as  part  of  the  activities  of  the  play  groups  and  gangs  of  which  he  was 
a  member.  It  is  clear  that  delinquency  was  an  established  tradition  in 
these  groups  prior  to  Sidney's  contact  with  them.  [51,  41] 

In  a  more  recent  publication,  Shaw  has  portrayed  the  individual 
cases  of  the  five  Martin  brothers  [521.  Their  social  and  economic 


INDIVIDUAL  MEMBERS  AS  INFLUENCED  BY  GROUP  NORMS  327 

examined  the  boys  concluded  that  they  could  be  properly  classified 
as  both  physically  and  mentally  normal.  Brothers  in  Crime  gives 
a  detailed  account  of  the  deviant  behavior  and  of  the  "settling 
down"  of  the  five  brothers  both  in  verified  objective  terms  and  in 
the  boys'  "own  stories."  The  general  lines  of  the  course  of  develop- 
ment of  their  psychological  identifications  and  attitudes  and  the 
factors  that  contributed  to  their  "settling  down"  in  society  at  large 
are  similar  to  those  summarized  briefly  in  the  case  of  the  Jack 
Roller.  In  short,  they  began  their  early  childhood  in  play  groups 
and  embryonic  gangs,  psychologically  identifying  themselves  with 
these  spontaneously  developed  groups. 

The  boys'  mother  was  a  devout  church  member.  But  as  the 
boys  became  involved  in  the  activities  of  their  gangs,  "they  repu- 
diated all  institutions  which  sought  to  enforce  conformity  to  con- 
ventional standards  of  conduct."  [140]  The  compellingness  of 
group  values  or  norms  in  shaping  the  brothers'  lives  was  indicated 
by  the  "inability  of  the  family,  the  school,  the  church,  the  juvenile 
court,  and  family  case-work  agencies  to  alter  their  conduct."  [126] 
Through  the  efforts  of  their  mother,  teachers,  social  workers,  and 
other  adults  the  boys  became  quite  familiar  with  the  norms  of  con- 
ventional society.  However,  knowledge  of  these  norms  did  not 
materially  affect  their  identifications,  attitudes,  and  behavior.  The 
boys  had  clearly  incorporated  the  values  and  norms  of  their  gangs 
as  their  own.  Consequently,  the  conflicting  norms  of  society  at 
large  had  little  or  no  effect  in  regulating  their  behavior. 

They  gravitated  towards  organic  membership  in  these  groups 
under  the  stress  of  basic  deprivations  and  the  denial  of  an  inte- 
grated personal  identity  in  home,  school,  church,  and  other  conven- 
tional situations.  They  had  to  derive  their  main  feelings  of  per- 
sonal status  from  membership  in  these  play  groups,  embryonic  or 
crystallized  gangs.  Their  basic  deprivations  were  relieved  through 
the  activities  of  these  groups.  Thus,  when  John  Martin,  the  oldest 
brother  who  set  the  example  for  the  others, 

.  .  .  was  approximately  seven  years  of  age,  he  became  identified 
with  one  of  the  many  flay  groups  in  his  community,  a  group  com- 
posed of  at  least  twelve  boys  ranging  in  age  from  five  to  twelve  years. 
Their  playgrounds  were  the  alleys,  streets,  and  railroad  yards;  their 
activities  were  largely  spontaneous,  random,  and  unsuper vised;  simple 


328  EGO  IDENTIHCATIONS  IN  GROUP  SITUATIONS 

forms  of  stealing  were  interspersed  with  nondeliquent  activities  with 
little  realization  of  their  moral  implications.  [52, 109,  italics  ours] 

The  psychiatrist  who  examined  the  Martin  brothers  reports  that 
they  "differed  in  regard  to  personality  traits."  But  "despite  differ- 
ences in  personality,  physical  stature,  and  intelligence,  all  of  the 
brothers  engaged  in  the  same  forms  of  delinquent  conduct 
throughout  the  early  periods  of  their  careers."  [325]  It  seems 
clear,  therefore,  that  the  similarity  of  their  behavior  in  early  years 
was  due  to  their  membership  in  similarly  formed  groups.  In 
later  adolescent  years  their  individually  deviant  behavior  differed 
according  to  the  specific  activities  of  the  gang  to  which  each, 
brother  belonged. 

One  of  Shaw's  generalizations  based  on  keen  observation  should 
be  of  particular  interest  to  any  psychologist  who  wants  to  see  the 
problems  of  personality  with  clear  perspective. 

It  is  interesting  to  observe  that  the  brothers  who  had  the  longest 
careers  in  delinquency  were  the  ones  who  possessed  personality  traits 
which  are  usually  regarded  as  being  most  desirable.  John,  Edward, 
James,  and  Michael  are  sociable,  friendly,  and  loyal  persons  who 
adapt  themselves  readily  to  other  individuals.  Carl,  on  the  other  hand, 
possesses  fewer  of  these  traits,  yet  he  continued  in  delinquency  for  a 
shorter  period  of  time  than  did  his  brothers.  It  is  suggested  that 
perhaps  socially  desirable  personality  traits  may  be  related  to  satisfac- 
tory adjustments  in  the  delinquent  group  in  the  same  manner  in  which 
they  are  related  to  adjustments  in  conventional  groups.  In  short, 
they  may  be  social  assets  in  both  situations.  Conversely  less  desirable 
traits  may  complicate  the  process  of  adjustment  in  delinquent  and  in 
nondelinquent  groups.  [52,  31 3] 

It  seems  a  tragic  irony,  in  the  case  of  the  Martin  brothers  at  least, 
that  a  less  sociable,  less  friendly,  and  less  loyal  "personality"  came 
as  a  blessing  to  save  one  of  the  brothers  from  the  more  prolonged 
indulgence  in  delinquent  behavior  of  his  more  sociable  brothers.12 

12  For  another  example,  see  case  1  in  fudge  Bafar  Foundation  Studies  No.  1 
(Boston,  1922).  Throughout  the  complete  report  of  this  delinquent  boy  there  are 
comments  by  various  officials  and  examiners  on  his  friendly  personality.  He  is 
described  as  "thoroughly  pleasant,  responsive,  smiling  often"  [26];  "fond  of 
activity  and  fun-loving"  [2#];  "friendly,  and  very  cooperative,  and  frank  in  his 
discussion."  [37a]  This  boy  had  nine  formal  complaints  against  him  in  three 
years'  time.  As  he  said,  "You  do  lots  of  things  to  make  others  think  you  are  a 
great  fellow."  [34] 


INDIVIDUAL  MEMBERS  AS  INFLUENCED  BY  GROUP  NORMS          329 

Shaw's  observations  of  the  family  origins  of  gang  members 
should  be  mentioned  for  they  give  further  insight  into  the  prob- 
lem of  parent-youth  conflict,  the  subsequent  psychological  drift- 
ing away  from  the  family  situation,  and  the  establishment  of  new 
ego  links.  So  far  the  gang  members  we  have  mentioned  all  came 
from  slum  areas.  However,  Shaw  states  that  there  are  cases  of 
youthful  gang  members  who  stem  from  "the  highest  income 
groups  in  the  community."  [101]  Such  cases  also  reflect  the  con- 
sequences of  parent-youth  friction  due  to  the  conflicting  identifica- 
tions of  children  and  their  parents.  As  we  have  seen  before, 
parents  and  boys  of  the  new  generation  may  have  incorporated 
values  or  norms  which  represent  either  different  or  contradictory 
cultural  patterns  due  in  some  instances  to  the  rapid  transition  of 
the  times. 

In  each  of  the  individual  studies,  we  have  seen  how  the  boy 
very  soon  embarked  once  more  on  delinquent  activities  after  he 
was  let  out  of  a  detention  home.  In  their  work  on  delinquency 
areas  in  Chicago,  Shaw  and  his  collaborators  [49]  give  significant 
hints  concerning  the  effect  of  group  formations  in  prolonging  the 
deviant  types  of  behavior  of  individual  group  members.  In  this 
provocative  work,  the  authors  systematically  studied  the  relative 
contribution  of  different  areas  of  the  city  to  the  total  number  of 
individuals  brought  to  court.  For  our  present  problem,  the  results 
on  "recidivism  among  male  delinquents  brought  before  the  juve- 
nile court  during  1900-1906  and  1917-1923"  are  important.  For 
among  other  things,  they  reflect  again  the  importance  of  group 
belongingness  or  group  reference  as  a  cause  of  the  differential  .rates 
of  recidivism  in  various  parts  of  the  city.  On  the  basis  of  their 
analysis,  the  authors  conclude: 

It  is  clear  from  the  foregoing  material  that  the  extent  of  recidivism 
is  highest  in  the  areas  having  the  highest  rate  of  individual  delinquents 
and  that  this  fact  explains  the  disproportionately  high  rate  of  delin- 
quency cases  in  these  areas.  This  finding  suggests  that  the  factors 
contributing  to  delinquency  in  these  areas  of  concentration  tend  also 
to  give  rise  to  recidivism  by  increasing  both  the  proportion  of  delin- 
quents who  become  recidivists  and  the  number  of  times  the  recidivists 
appear  in  court.  [49, 186] 


330  EGO  IDENTIHCATIONS  IN  GROUP  SITUATIONS 

In  a  more  recent  work,  Shaw  and  McKay  [53],  using  similar 
statistics  collected  from  1927  to  1933,  reach  the  same  conclusion. 

From  these  findings  and  other  material  on  gangs  already  cited, 
it  is  clear  that  continued  gang  membership  is  one  of  the  chief 
factors  in  the  relatively  high  rate  of  recidivism  in  these  areas. 
There  is  concrete  evidence  that,  if  young  delinquents  after  their 
release  from  prison  could  be  placed  in  informal  groups  where  their 
interests  could  be  directed  along  other  channels  and  where  they 
could  feel  they  were  active  participants,  the  rate  of  recidivism 
would  decrease.  Referring  to  such  attempts  undertaken  under 
the  supervision  of  Clifford  Shaw  in  certain  Chicago  areas,  Martin 
[30]  writes: 

Shaw  believes  it  [recidivism]  is  caused  partially  by  the  tendency  of 
an  ex-convict's  community  to  shun  him  when  he  comes  out  of  prison. 
Barred  this  contact  with  "respectable"  people,  the  ex-con  is  forced  to 
seek  association  with  criminals.  Soon  he  will  go  back  to  prison.  The 
Area  Project  attempts  to  reintegrate  ex-cons  into  their  communities. 
The  parolees  handled  by  the  Russell  Square  Committee  range  from 
youngsters  of,  say,  fourteen  years,  locked  up  for  the  first  time,  to  men 
of  fifty  with  long  criminal  records.  Of  forty-seven  parolees  dealt  with, 
only  one  has  been  returned  to  prison  for  parole  violation.  (The  parole 
agent  caught  him  drunk.)  [30] 

In  this  connection,  the  practical  remarks  contained  in  a  recent 
memorandum  from  Shaw  to  the  board  of  directors  of  the  Chicago 
Area  Project  are  pertinent.  [54]  They  imply  the  psychological  fact 
that  the  direction  of  an  individual's  behavior  is  not  affected  in  a 
major  way  unless  and  until  he  becomes  ego-involved  in  social 
situations.  Shaw  observed: 

Attempts  to  produce  these  changes  for  the  community  by  means  of 
ready-made  institutions  and  programs  planned,  developed,  financed, 
and  managed  by  persons  outside  the  community  are  not  likely  to  meet 
with  any  more  success  in  the  future  than  they  have  in  the  past.  This 
procedure  is  psychologically  unsound  because  it  places  the  residents 
of  the  community  in  an  inferior  position  and  implies  serious  reserva- 
tions with  regard  to  their  capacities  and  their  interest  in  their  own 
welfare.  What  is  equally  important  is  the  fact  that  it  neglects  the 
greatest  of  all  assets  in  any  community,  namely  the  talents,  energies, 
and  other  human  resources  of  the  people  themselves.  For  these 


SOME  PSYCHOLOGICAL  LEADS  331 

reasons,  these  superimposed  programs  and  institutions,  while  perhaps 
providing  temporary  aid  to  individuals  and  families,  are  not  likely  to 
exercise  any  deep  and  lasting  influence  in  the  social  character  of  the 
community.  They  are  related  to  the  community  only  in  the  most 
superficial  manner  and  thus  have  not  appreciably  reduced  the  volume 
of  truancy,  delinquency,  and  crime.  What  is  necessary,  we  believe,  is 
the  organization  and  encouragement  of  social  self-help  on  a  coopera- 
tive basis.  [54,  2] 

SOME  PSYCHOLOGICAL  LEADS 

The  conclusions  reached  on  the  basis  of  the  material  so  far 
reviewed  are  further  confirmed  by  the  more  psychological  analysis 
of  Healy  and  Bronner  [19].  In  their  study  of  many  delinquent 
cases  who  were  paired  with  nondelinquents,  die  authors  investi- 
gate from  all  possible  angles  the  factors  contributing  to  deviant 
types  of  behavior.  Without  denying  in  the  least  the  contribution 
of  other  variables,  we  shall  call  attention  to  the  impact  of  group 
contact  in  producing  individual  delinquency.  We  repeat  that  our 
concern  throughout  this  chapter  has  been  the  group  determination 
of  this  consequential  type  of  behavior.  For  this  throws  light  on 
group  behavior  in  general.  We  have  not  been  interested  in  indi- 
vidual or  group  delinquency  as  such. 

The  chain  of  causation  between  basic  urges,  desires,  and  wishes 
on  the  one  hand,  and  delinquency  on  the  other,  runs  clear  through 
Healy  and  Bronner's  impressive  work.  The  authors  have  clearly 
diagrammed  this  chain  in  Figure  3.18  We  see  that  basic  urges, 
desires,  and  wishes  may  be  satisfied  through  "socially  acceptable 
activities"  in  society  whatever  the  particular  norms  of  the  particu- 
lar society  may  be.  If  they  are  not  satisfied,  if  they  are  hindered 
by  economic  or  other  situational  circumstances,  "feelings  of  inade- 
quacy, deprivations,  and  thwartings"  arise.  These  in  turn  are 
followed  by  urges  for  substitute  satisfactions.  These  urges  for 
substitute  satisfactions  need  not  necessarily  express  themselves  in 
delinquent  behavior.  Between  these  urges  and  actual  participation 
in  delinquent  activity  there  is  the  step  of  "acceptance  of  ideas  of 
delinquency."  In  the  authors'  words: 

18  Reproduced  from  [19,  4]  by  permission. 


332 


EGO  IDENTIFICATIONS  IN  GROUP  SITUATIONS 


Now  what  form  substitutive  activities  will  take,  whether  or  not 
they  will  be  antisocial,  depends  partly  on  external  circumstances,  but 
mainly  upon  the  acceptance  of  certain  ideas.  [19,  7] 

The  process  of  acceptance  is  further  elaborated  in  such  formula- 
tions: 

Very  few  indeed,  if  any,  enter  into  delinquency  as  the  result  of  a 
new  born  impulse,  without  previously  having  had  thought  about  it. 


I  ACCEPTANCE  OF         — 

»        IDEAS  OF  DELINQUENCY    — 


FIG.  3.    Diagram  showing  the  development  of  delinquent  behavior.    The  general 
life  stream  of  feelings  and  activities. 

There  is  almost  universally  some  period  of  incubation  of  such  ideas, 
generally  not  thought  out  to  definite  conclusions  concerning  action, 
but  still  recurring  as  part  of  the  mental  content.  An  educative  or 
assimilative  process  has  been  going  on,  usually  under  the  stimulus  of 
some  environmental  source  or  sources,  whereby  the  individual  learns 
about  delinquency,  its  forms  and  techniques,  as  he  might  become 
informed  or  educated  about f  we  will  say,  tree  climbing.  [19,  68,  italics 
ours] 

The  impact  of  group  factors  in  the  "acceptance  of  ideas  of 
delinquency"  is  stressed  time  and  again  in  different  connections  by 
Healy  and  Bronner.  For  example,  they  state  that  "litdc  delin- 
quency is  engaged  in  without  companionship  or  gang  connections" 
[63,  italics  ours]  And  again,  "this  particular  educative  process 
generally  began  with  information  received  from  youthful  com- 
rades— two  were  taught  by  older  criminals."  [66] 


SOME  PSYCHOLOGICAL  LEADS  333 

In  our  opinion,  the  process  of  acceptance  of  ideas  or  norms  of 
delinquency  through  social  contacts  will  gain  clarity  and  func- 
tional usefulness  if  it  is  conceptualized  in  terms  of  identifications 
in  actual  groups  or  ego-involvements  in  relation  to  certain  refer- 
ence groups  in  general  and  membership  groups  in  particular. 
Once  identification  takes  place,  whatever  the  size  of  the  group 
may  be,  essentially  similar  psychological  consequences  may  be 
expected.  Psychologically,  it  makes  a  great  deal  of  difference 
whether  praise  or  ridicule  comes  from  an  indifferent  group  or  a 
group  in  which  we  are  strongly  ego-involved.  Once  the  norms  of 
a  delinquent  group  or  any  other  group  are  accepted,  once  identi- 
fication with  a  group  takes  place,  the  individual  is  not  necessarily 
content  merely  to  hold  the  attitudes  of  the  group  or  to  indulge  in 
prescribed  activities.  He  may  spontaneously  praise  the  attitudes, 
spontaneously  engage  in  new  forms  of  behavior  consistent  with 
the  group  line. 

The  material  gathered  by  Healy  and  Bronner  indicates  clearly 
the  way  a  boy  may  become  identified  with  a  group  and  the 
psychological  consequences  of  this  identification. 

A  boy,  for  example,  feeling  himself  inadequate  in  other  relation- 
ships finds  himself  accepted  and  gains  recognition  with  a  gang  if  he 
takes  up  with  the  suggestions  they  give  him  of  stealing  with  or  for 
them.  [19,  8] 

They  mention  in  passing  the  case  of  a  delinquent  boy  who  "previ- 
ously was  effeminate  or  feminized."  The  boy  gave  the  rationale 
of  his  deviant  behavior. 

They  thought  I  was  no  good  so  I  went  out  to  show  a  cockeyed 
world  that  I  was  a  regular  guy.  [19, 134] 

Although  the  authors  do  not  specify  it,  the  "cockeyed  world"  the 
boy  referred  to  is  undoubtedly  the  reference  group  whose  ridicule 
at  last  drove  him  to  such  action,  and  in  relation  to  which  he 
wanted  to  excel  as  a  "regular  guy." 

In  Shaw's  account  of  the  Martin  brothers,  we  noted  that  the 
brothers  who  engaged  in  delinquent  activities  the  longest  Were 
the  most  sociable  friendly  ones.  In  this  connection,  it  is  interesting 
to  note  that  Healy  and  Bronner  found  that  "gregariousness"  or 


334  EGO  IDENTIFICATIONS  IN  GROUP  SITUATIONS 

"sociability"  is  clearly  more  prevalent  among  the  delinquents  than 
among  the  individuals  used  as  controls.  The  greater  sociability  of 
delinquents  may  be,  at  least  in  part,  a  product  of  their  striving  to 
anchor  themselves  in  a  social  setting  when  social  status  is  denied 
them  by  already  established  groups.  The  study  of  Healy  and 
Bronner  clearly  suggests  the  psychological  function  served  by  the 
opportunity  to  anchor  one's  self  in  a  definite  group,  even  though 
it  may  be  a  delinquent  one.  For  example: 

Under  certain  circumstances  it  may  be  healthier  and  more  normal 
to  join  in  with  the  activities  and  imbibe  the  ideas  of  a  delinquent 
crowd  than  to  be  a  withdrawing,  soft,  effeminate  "mother's  boy"  or, 
as  in  instances  already  mentioned,  to  mope  at  home  and  develop  an 
abnormal  phantasy  life.  [19,  136] 

SOME  VERIFICATIONS  OF  THE  EFFECTS  OF  GROUP  SITUATIONS 

Some  representative  data  already  presented  have  demonstrated 
the  effects  of  group  situations  on  individuals.  We  have  seen  how 
ego-attitudes  may  be  structured  or  modified  in  group  situations  in 
a  lasting  or  a  temporary  way.  During  the  past  two  decades,  these 
findings  have  been  amply  verified  on  the  psychological  level.  We 
shall  limit  our  discussion  here  to  a  brief  summary  of  a  few  investi- 
gations. All  of  them  compel  us  to  call  attention  once  more  to  the 
convergence  of  sociological  and  psychological  formulations.  They 
prove  that  there  cannot  be  two  kinds  of  psychologies  of  collective 
behavior:  the  psychology  of  the  individual  in  group  situations  is 
valid  social  psychology,  and  his  social  psychology  is  valid  individual 
psychology. 

Since  social  interaction  is  possible  only  when  individuals  in  a 
situation  are  able  to  grasp  reciprocal  relationships  (the  point  of 
view  of  others  as  well  as  their  own)  and  when  certain  rules  or 
norms  established  as  a  consequence  of  human  interaction  in  the 
past  or  emerging  in  the  actual  situation  are  observed,  the  studies  of 
Piaget  and  his  associates,  carried  out  in  the  1920's  are  especially 
impressive.  These  investigations  conclusively  elucidate  the  genetic 
development  and  properties  of  group  interactions  and  furnish  an 
unique  crucible  of  the  psychology  of  group  interaction,  the  incor- 


SOME  VERIFICATIONS  OF  THE  EFFECTS  OF  GROUP  SITUATIONS       335 

poration  of  social  norms  in  the  individual,  and  their  subsequent 
effects  on  behavior. 

Piaget  studied  the  successive  stages  of  development  of  (1)  the 
practice  of  rules  and  (2)  the  consciousness  of  rules.  Since  his 
results  show  that  development  runs  parallel  in  both  respects,  we 
shall  not  make  a  special  point  of  presenting  his  results  separately 
and  shall  mention  only  those  findings  most  relevant  to  our  dis- 
cussion. He  specifically  examined  the  development  of  the  indi- 
vidual's morality  from  the  comparatively  early  years  of  childhood 
almost  up  to  puberty.  As  he  puts  it,  "All  morality  consists  in  a 
system  of  rules,  and  the  essence  of  all  morality  is  to  be  sought  for 
in  the  respect  which  the  individual  acquires  for  these  rules."  [45,7] 
After  calling  attention  to  this  general  fact,  Piaget  starts  with  a 
detailed  study  of  "the  rules  of  the  game"  as  played  among  age 
mates.  These  "rules  of  the  game"  exemplified  by  the  game  of 
marbles,  are  among  the  first  rules  grasped  by  the  developing  child. 

During  the  first  years  of  his  life  (in  the  motor  stage)  the  child 
cannot  really  participate  in  a  play  situation,  let  alone  in  more 
developed  levels  of  group  participation.  This  inability  of  the  very 
young  child  to  share  collective  activity  of  the  simplest  sort  is  due 
to  the  fact  that  his  ego  is  not  sufficiently  differentiated  for  him  to 
distinguish  between  what  is  his  own  desire  and  what  is  external 
to  him.  He  cannot  yet  separate  fantasy  and  fact.  In  short,  he 
cannot  recognize  his  ever-changing  mercurial  subjectivity  as  such. 
Hence,  he  cannot  consistently  follow  a  conversation,  a  game,  or 
any  social  activity  involving  a  give-and-take  type  of  relationship. 
As  he  grows  older,  certain  rules  (norms)  are  imposed  on  him  by 
parents,  older  boys  and  girls,  or  other  grown-ups.  He  is  made  to 
abide  by  these  rules  in  the  daily  routine  of  life.  But  even  though 
he  abides  by  them,  for  the  child  around  5  or  6  years  of  age,  rules 
or  norms  have  not  yet  become  an  integrated  part  of  his  psychology. 
To  him,  the  rules  or  norms  he  is  made  to  follow  are  absolute,  made 
in  an  unchangeable  way  by  grown-ups.  Even  though  he  abides 
by  them  because  of  the  sheer  constraint  and  authority  exerted  by 
his  elders,  he  easily  lapses  to  the  ever-changing  flux  of  his  desires 
and  fantasy  when  he  is  not  under  the  grip  of  external  authority. 
For  these  externally  imposed  rules  or  norms  have  not  yet  become 
his  own  rules  or  norms. 


336  EGO  IDENTIFICATIONS  IN  GROUP  SITUATIONS 

Only  after  further  development  (around  the  age  of  7-8  in  the 
case  of  Piaget's  children)  is  the  child  mature  enough  to  grasp 
consistently  the  points  of  view  of  others,  to  interact  reciprocally 
and  to  co-operate  in  play  and  other  activities  with  his  equals  (age 
mates).  As  a  consequence  of  age-mate  group  interactions  and 
co-operations,  the  group  rules  or  norms  become  his  own  norms. 
In  this  process  he  ceases  to  consider  them  as  the  absolute  and 
immutable  dictates  of  grown-ups.  In  the  course  of  the  develop- 
ment of  participation  in  age-mate  activities  (around  the  age  of 
11-12  in  Piaget's  cases),  children  reach  the  stage  where  they  col- 
lectively and  spontaneously  evolve  and  codify  appropriate  norms 
for  the  situation  at  hand. 

These  spontaneously  emerging  norms  in  collective  age-mate 
activities  are  among  the  first  norms  that  boys  or  girls  accept,  re- 
spect, and  abide  by  as  their  own.  At  this  point  the  experience  and 
behavior  of  co-operation  and  autonomy  as  contrasted  to  authority 
and  heteronomy  is  first  indicated.  In  Piaget's  words: 

From  the  moment  that  children  really  begin  to  submit  to  rules  and 
to  apply  them  in  a  spirit  of  genuine  cooperation,  they  acquire  a  new 
conception  of  these  rules.  Rules  become  something  that  can  be 
changed  if  it  is  agreed  that  they  should  be,  for  the  truth  of  a  rule  docs 
not  rest  on  tradition  but  on  mutual  agreement  and  reciprocity.  [89] 
Henceforward,  he  will  not  only  discover  the  boundaries  that  separate 
his  self  from  the  other  person,  but  will  learn  to  understand  the  other 
person  and  be  understood  by  him.  So  that  cooperation  is  really  a 
factor  in  the  creation  of  personality,  if  by  personality  we  mean,  not 
the  unconscious  self  of  childish  egocentrism,  but  the  self  that  takes 
up  its  stand  on  the  norms  of  reciprocity  and  objective  discussion,  and 
knows  how  to  submit  to  these  in  order  to  make  itself  respected.  .  .  . 
Cooperation  being  the  source  of  personality,  rules  cease,  in  accordance 
with  the  same  principle,  to  be  external.  They  become  both  the  con- 
stitutive factors  of  personality  and  its  fruit,  in  accordance  with  the 
circular  process  so  frequently  exemplified  in  the  course  of  mental 
development.  In  this  way  autonomy  succeeds  heteronomy.  [45,  90] 

Likewise,  with  the  development  of  co-operative  group  activities, 
the  unilateral  respect  and  awe  felt  towards  grown-ups  and  the 
conforming  behavior  of  early  childhood  owing  to  the  pressure  of 
external  authority  alone  give  way  to  feelings  of  mutual  respect 


SOME  VERIFICATIONS  OF  THE  EFFECTS  OF  GROUP  SITUATIONS       337 

and  "inner  responsibility ."  These  conclusions  based  on  rich  con- 
crete data  and  so  briefly,  summarized  here,  are  full  of  implications 
concerning  the  effects  of  group  situations  in  determining  an  indi- 
vidual's personal  values  and  loyalties.  They  substantiate  on  a  more 
refined  level  of  psychological  analysis  the  evidence  that  emerged 
from  the  gang  studies  carried  out  at  approximately  the  same  period 
in  a  different  country.  (See  pp.  289-304.) 

Piaget  considers  the  social  and  practical  implications  of  his 
findings  in  the  final  section  of  his  study.  The  upshot  of  his  bril- 
liant discussion  is  that  the  individual  can  only  develop  a  sense  of 
the  reciprocal  nature  of  real  human  relationships  through  partici- 
pation in  group  activities.  Only  through  such  co-operative  par- 
ticipation does  he  come  to  accept  group  norms  as  his  own  and  to 
develop  his  identification,  loyalty,  and  iqner  responsibility  towards 
them.  If  such  co-operative  participation  is  lacking,  the  individual 
considers  the  norms  imposed  on  him  by  sheer  authority  as  nui- 
sances to  be  evaded  whenever  possible.  Only  as  an  outcome  of 
co-operation  in  group  situations  with  equals  do  "the  infantile  traits 
that  mark  the  conformist  spirit  make  place  for  the  features  that 
are  the  outcome  of  cooperation,"  [341]  "Whence  the  decline  of 
unilateral  respect  and  the  primacy  of  personal  judgment.  But  in 
consequence  of  this,  cooperation  suppresses  both  egocentrism  and 
moral  realism,  and  thus  achieves  an  interiorization  of  rules."  [411] 
At  the  end,  Piaget  makes  explicit  the  implications  of  his  results: 
"It  is  obvious  that  our  results  are  as  unfavourable  to  the  method  of 
authority  as  to  purely  individualistic  methods."  Piaget  specifically 
criticized  the  authoritarian  sociology  of  Durkheim  and  its  educa- 
tional deductions. 

Unfortunately,  under  the  influence  of  a  "pre-notion,"  hard  to 
account  for  in  a  sociologist,  and  especially  in  one  so  methodical, 
Durkheim  thinks  of  children  as  knowing  no  other  society  than  adult 
society  or  the  society  created  by  adults  (schools),  so  that  he  entirely 
ignores  the  existence  of  spontaneously  formed  children's  societies,  and 
of  the  facts  relating  to  mutual  respect.  Consequently,  elastic  though 
Durkheim's  pedagogy  may  be  in  principle,  it  simply  leads,  for  lack  of 
being  sufficiently  informed  on  the  subject  of  child  sociology,  to  a 
defence  of  the  methods  of  authority.  [45,  358  f.] 


338  EGO  IDENTIFICATIONS  IN  GROUP  SITUATIONS 

Unlike  psychologists  who  complacently  stop  after  uttering  their 
conclusions  on  the  basis  of  restricted  "closed"  situations,  Piaget 
anxiously  looks  ahead  to  see  if  his  conclusions  hold  in  concrete 
situations: 

For,  after  all,  it  is  one  thing  to  prove  that  cooperation  in  the  play 
and  spontaneous  social  life  of  children  brings  about  certain  moral 
effects,  and  another  to  establish  the  fact  that  this  cooperation  can  be 
universally  applied  as  a  method  of  education.  This  last  point  is  one 
which  only  experimental  education  can  settle.  Educational  experi- 
ment, on  condition  that  it  be  scientifically  controlled,  is  certainly  more 
instructive  for  psychology  than  any  amount  of  laboratory  experiments, 
and  because  of  this  experimental  pedagogy  might  perhaps  be  incor- 
porated into  the  body  of  the  psychosociological  disciplines.  [45,  413  f.] 

The  results  of  educational  processes  which  give  an  opportunity 
for  free  co-operation  among  equals  in  diverse  activities  and  which 
encourage  initiative  in  positive  group  undertakings  are  described 
in  many  reports  by  progressive  educators.  An  outstanding  illus- 
tration of  the  emergent  effects  of  democratically  organized  collec- 
tive training  on  individuals  is  seen  in  Makarenko's  far-reaching 
work  [6].  In  1920,  at  a  time  when  famine  and  civil  war  were  still 
ravaging  the  Soviet  Union,  Makarenko  started  the  Gorki  Colony, 
a  school  devoted  to  the  reforming  and  training  of  juvenile  delin- 
quents. The  first  six  students  who  arrived  at  the  colony  were,  for 
example,  youngsters  who  had  committed  quite  serious  offenses. 
"Two  of  them  had  been  found  guilty  of  larcenies,  four  of  armed 
burglaries.  In  the  institution  they  acted  as  a  unit.  .  .  .  They  as- 
sumed an  attitude  of  indifference  to  the  rules  or  to  the  staff."  [38] 

After  Makarenko  had  once  proved  to  the  youngsters  that  he 
could  be  tough  too,  he  devoted  his  energies  to  the  creation  of  a 
common  life  in  which  administration,  teachers,  and  students  alike 
participated.  To  this  end,  they  all  shared  in  all  activities  and 
hardships  and  in  any  rewards  that  came  as  a  consequence  of 
common  effort.  In  Makarenko's  words:  "In  our  nervewracking 
poverty  there  was  one  good  feature — all  of  us,  teachers  and  pupils, 
were  hungry  and  poor  alike."  The  report  continues:  "Thus  in 
the  process  of  living  together,  a  common  formula  was  being 
shaped.  Events  molded  both  director  and  pupils,  forming  the 


SOME  VERIFICATIONS  OF  THE  EFFECTS  OF  GROUP  SITUATIONS      339 

rudiments  of  a  community  interest."  [6,  39]  In  this  collective 
process  of  working  and  living  together,  which  effectively  trans- 
formed the  attitudes  of  the  participating  individuals,  stealing, 
drinking,  and  gambling  which  were  common  when  the  institution 
started,  were  successfully  eliminated.  Once  the  collective  trend  of 
the  colony  took  definite  shape,  all  such  objectionable  behavior  was 
eliminated,  not  through  the  use  of  punishments,  but  by  putting  the 
matter  to  the  students  themselves  and  securing  their  active  par- 
ticipation. The  positive  results  emerging  in  the  Gorki  Colony 
were  carried  to  other  such  institutions.  In  these  projects  the  stu- 
dents of  the  Gorki  Colony  played  no  small  part.  From  the  origi- 
nal colony  and  another  carried  to  its  same  level  by  the  Gorki 
teachers  and  students,  three  thousand  boys  and  girls  graduated. 
Many  of  them  attained  leading  positions  in  industry,  the  arts,  and 
sciences.  When  they  assumed  responsible  work  in  society  at  large, 
they  were  not  treated  as  exconvicts  or  "reformed"  boys,  but  as 
full-fledged  citizens. 

It  may  be  safe  to  conclude  that  the  success  of  this  educational 
experiment  in  the  Soviet  Union  in  transforming  the  whole  life 
patterns  of  unfortunate  deviant  boys  and  girls  into  highly  useful 
citizens  is  due  to  the  fact  that  the  conception  and  character  of  the 
experiment  fit  in  with  and,  in  fact,  were  encouraged  by  the  whole 
social  system.  During  the  first  decade  of  the  century,  exponents  of 
progressive  education  in  many  countries  spread  their  enthusiasm 
for  creating  an  ideal  society  through  the  education  of  new  genera- 
tions. And,  when  we  consider  the  Gorki  Colony  experiment  in 
the  light  of  similar  undertakings  in  capitalist  countries,  when  we 
relate  it  to  the  disillusionment  expressed  by  many  progressive 
educators  in  Europe  and  America,  its  implications  become  even 
more  striking.  The  fate  of  so  many  experiments  in  progressive 
education  tends  to  show  that,  in  highly  differentiated  Western 
societies  where  individuals  must  sooner  or  later  face  groups  with 
contradictory  conflicting  functions  and  values,  they  are  bound  to 
be  torn  in  different  directions  with  inevitable  psychological  con- 
sequences. Before  jumping  to  any  conclusions  about  the  definite 
effects  of  a  particular  group  on  its  individual  members,  it  therefore 
becomes  necessary  to  investigate  at  the  same  time  the  question  of 
whether  or  not  the  groups  which  an  individual  sooner  or  later 


340  EGO  IDENTIFICATIONS  IN  GROUP  SITUATIONS 

will  face  in  a  major  way  are  integrated  in  function  and  values. 
For  groups  cannot  be  taken  as  closed  systems,  especially  today. 
The  values  and  directions  shaped  in  one  group  have  serious  conse- 
quences in  the  lives  of  an  individual  member  if  they  run  counter 
to  those  of  other  groups  which  he  is  subsequently  bound  to  en- 
counter. It  is  not  difficult  to  find  cases  of  antagonistic  groups  in 
which  the  degree  of  in-group  solidarity,  sympathy  and  co-opera- 
tion may  be  proportional  to  the  prejudices,  intolerance,  and  enmity 
exhibited  towards  the  members  of  out-groups.  For  example,  the 
utmost  degree  of  consideration,  democracy,  and  good  will  ex- 
hibited within  the  inner  bounds  of  an  exclusive  club  or  a  Junior 
League  need  not  be  bestowed  on  out-group  persons,  especially  if 
the  persons  in  question  are  members  of  groups  standing  at  some 
distance  from  the  club's  particular  social  hierarchy. 

Other  relevant  experimental  and  research  studies  of  groups  have 
been  reported  which  provide  substantial  verification  of  the  effects 
of  group  situations  on  the  individual  member's  identifications  and 
ego  relationships,  on  the  structural  properties  of  groups,  and  on  the 
effects  of  differential  group  identifications.  Here  we  can  mention 
only  a  few  representative  studies,  among  which  should  be  included 
the  work  of  Moreno  and  his  associates.  Before  1925,  Moreno 
found  that  individuals  assigned  in  a  group  to  improvise  "certain 
attitudes  for  typical  situations"  often  committed  "the  most  bewil- 
dering acts,  although  in  the  realm  of  fiction,  which  seem  unrelated 
to  their  individual  selves  when  we  saw  them  in  daily  life."  [33, 13] 
In  1934,  Moreno  [35]  summarized  a  considerable  body  of  research 
chiefly  concerned  with  interpersonal  relationships  within  various 
groups.  For  example,  by  asking  every  child  from  kindergarten 
through  the  eighth  grade  in  a  public  school  to  choose  seat  mates, 
Moreno  constructed  "sociograms"  showing  such  social  relations 
within  each  class.  He  found  a  progressive  development  and  differ- 
entiation in  these  children's  groups.  In  kindergarten  and  the  first 
and  second  grades,  children  were  "seldom  sufficiently  certain 
whom  to  choose!'  [35,  54,  italics  ours]  After  about  the  fourth 
grade,  intragroup  relationships  were  more  complex,  suggesting 
that  children  develop  to  "exchange  emotions  readily  and  freely 
form  partnerships  and  secret  associations."  [35,  54]  With  the 
development  of  "cooperative  group  action,"  with  the  "increased 


SOME  VERIFICATIONS  OF  THE  EFFECTS  OF  GROUP  SITUATIONS      341 

differentiation"  of  children's  groups,  a  "cleavage"  from  adults 
occurs,  as  evidenced  by  "the  declining  insight  of  adults"  into  the 
relationships  within  children's  groups.  [35,  50]  Only  after  recip- 
rocal relationships  developed  among  children  did  they  accurately 
practice  status  distinctions  on  the  basis  of  grown-up  norms.  For 
example,  after  about  the  fourth  grade  "the  percentage  of  hetero- 
sexual attractions  drops  very  low."  [35,  61]  And,  at  about  the 
same  time,  choices  were  made  more  frequently  in  terms  of  the 
children's  own  nationality  and  color  groups  with  simultaneous 
rejection  of  outside  groups  as  prescribed  by  grown-up  norms. 
"This  phenomenon  could  not  be  observed  in  the  pre-school  groups 
nor  in  the  1st,  2nd,  and  3rd  grades  although  the  percentage  of 
members  of  different  nationalities  was  about  the  same."  [35,  61] 
The  effects  of  relatively  unified  groups  as  contrasted  to  relatively 
divided  groups  are  shown  in  Moreno's  study  of  the  New  York 
State  Training  School  for  Girls.  The  standard  of  conduct  and 
morale  of  the  group  and  the  number  of  runaways  and  requests  for 
transfers  varied  with  the  degree  of  interest  in  the  group. 

Moreno  and  his  associates  have  developed  ingenious  devices, 
such  as  the  sociogram,  and  statistical  refinements  which  facilitate 
the  study  of  groups.  [37]  Considerable  research  has  been  stimu- 
lated. To  take  a  recent  example,  Bronfenbrenner  [10]  essentially 
repeated  Moreno's  studies  of  school  children.  One  of  the  signifi- 
cant findings  of  this  study  was  that  the  shift  in  an  individual's 
status  "must  be  interpreted  not  only  in  terms  of  change  in  indi- 
vidual, social,  or  psychological  adjustment,  but  also  in  terms  of 
group  developmental  trends."  [76] 

However,  the  theoretical  background  presented  by  Moreno  puts 
the  psychologist  in  the  puzzling  situation  of  studying  empirical 
data  along  with  theoretical  formulations  mixed  with  mystic  no- 
tions. For  example,  in  1943  Moreno  [36]  wrote:  "I  have  had  the 
good  fortune  to  develop  three  ideas.  The  first  idea,  a  study  of  the 
godhead,  has  remained  cryptic  and  misapprehended.  The  second, 
a  study  of  man  called  psychodrama,  has  aroused  some  hope  that 
man  can  train  his  spontaneity  to  overcome  many  of  his  short- 
comings. My  third  idea,  the  study  of  society  called  sociometry, 
has  given  the  greatest  promise  that  a  measure  can  be  developed 
for  a  deeper  understanding  of  society  and  a  key  to  the  treatment 


342  EGO  IDENTIFICATIONS  IN  GROUP  SITUATIONS 

of  its  ills.  Many  of  my  friends  consider  these  three  ideas  one 
apart  from  the  other.  In  my  own  mind,  however,  all  three  ideas 
are  of  one  piece.  One  has  developed  out  of  the  other."  [299]  The 
"study  of  the  godhead"  is  expressed  in  The  Words  of  the  Father 
[34],  a  book  published  in  1941  and  listed  as  one  of  the  "Basic 
Works  of  J.  L.  Moreno"  in  Sociometry.  The  preface  and  com- 
mentary of  the  book  are  provided  by  Moreno  who  points  out 
that  "on  the  title  page  of  this  book  there  is  a  vacant  place:  there 
is  no  author's  name  there."  [316]  "The  name  of  the  author  is 
given  in  the  title  itself — in  The  Words  of  the  Father.  [317]  As 
Moreno  points  out  in  the  beginning  of  his  preface  "This  is  an 
extraordinary  book."  [vii]  He  continues: 

It  is  unique  because  of  its  premise.  Even  before  a  line  of  it  is 
read— before  it  is  weighted  as  to  its  content— its  premise  is:  these 
are  God's  own  words,  [vii]  .  .  .  God  Himself,  not  only  speaks  but 
is  shown  acting,  creating,  ruling  and  judging.  God  is  present.  He 
creates  His  own  universe.  He  analyzes  His  own  universe.  He  is  in 
direct  communication  with  every  atom  of  the  universe,  [ix]  ...  It 
would  seem  that  the  idea  of  God  has  found  in  this  book  a  final  and 
total  expression,  [x]  .  .  .  It  is  a  consolation  that  the  message  of  the 
Father  comes  to  the  world  at  this  time.  There  has  never  been  a 
moment  when  a  word  of  encouragement  was  more  welcome,  [xxi] 

God's  pronouncements  cover  over  100  pages.    The  first  reads: 

I  AM  GOD, 

THE  FATHER, 

THE  CREATOR  OF  THE  UNIVERSE. 

THESE  ARE  MY  WORDS, 

THE  WORDS  OF  THE  FATHER. 

Such  prophetic  and  ecstatic  statements  are  hardly  acceptable  to 
the  psychologist. 

In  1926,  the  Wawokiye  Camp  Research  Project  [40],  a  study  in 
"experimental  sociology,"  began  under  the  direction  of  W.  I. 
Newstetter  of  Western  Reserve  University,  Designed  to  gain 
"generalized  knowledge  of  the  primary  group  and  the  means  for 
its  study"  [93],  this  long-range  study,  in  the  words  of  its  director, 
"provides  devastating  evidence  that  interactions  (social  behavior) 


SOME  VERIFICATIONS  OF  THE  EFFECTS  OF  GROUP  SITUATIONS       343 

are  not  the  result  of  measurable  'traits.' "  [104]  During  the  early 
period  of  the  project  (1926-28),  the  approach  was  slanted  chiefly 
towards  the  diagnosis  and  therapeutics  of  the  individual  in  social 
situations.  "The  value  of  adjustment  through  group  association 
was  demonstrated  by  practical  results."  \vt\  However,  research 
results  were  not  so  satisfactory.  "The  measures  of  these  supposed 
traits  weighted  in  some  way  were  intended  to  yield  a  score  which 
was  taken  as  an  index  of  social  adjustment.  This  may  be  a  suitable 
approach  for  the  interpretation  of  individual  reactions,  but  has 
been  unfruitful  as  the  basis  for  interpreting  social  behavior."  [93  /.] 

As  a  result,  the  orientation  turned  toward  a  study  of  the  group 
itself  as  a  step  forward  in  achieving  a  "situational  approach." 
From  1930  to  1933,  two  five-week  camp  periods  were  held  each 
summer.  About  thirty  boys,  most  of  them  problem  cases,  attended 
each  session.  Camp  life  was  more  or  less  unplanned,  activity  and 
direction  generating  chiefly  from  the  boys  themselves.  Among 
the  findings  relevant  to  our  discussion  were  the  measurements  of 
status  based  on  weekly  reports  of  preference  for  companions.  By 
correlating  these  indices  of  group  status,  it  was  found  that  each 
group  had  a  more  or  less  characteristic  pattern  of  stability  in  status 
relationships  from  week  to  week. 

An  attempt  to  discern  what  sort  of  behavior  is  directly  related  to 
acceptance  by  the  group  was  undertaken  by  Newcomb.  [40,  66-92] 
As  a  result  of  considerable  previous  experience,  cordial  and  an- 
tagonistic responses  were  chosen  as  being  most  inclusive  and  re- 
lated to  the  problem.  Observation  of  free  groupings  was  recorded 
on  a  nine-point  scale  with  considerable  reliability.  The  principal 
finding  of  this  observation  was  that  cordiality  received  was  sig- 
nificantly related  to  group  status,  while  cordiality  (or  antagonism) 
given  bore  no  significant  relation  to  group  status.  Attempts  to 
relate  ascendance-submission,  volubility  of  expression,  and  atten- 
tion to  counselors  given  by  a  member  to  his  status  revealed  no 
relationship  between  the  measures,  although  ascendance-submis- 
sion received  was  related  to  group  status.  "Our  principle  conclu- 
sion, then,  is  a  methodological  or  technical  one.  Significant  and 
reliable  measures  involving  interactions  of  individuals  can  be  ob- 
tained from  behavior  observations,  if  these  interactions  are  not 
assumed  to  flow  from  traits  of  individuals."  [92] 


344  EGO  IDENTIFICATIONS  IN  GROUP  SITUATIONS 

Another  valuable  series  of  studies  stems  from  Lewin  and  his 
students.  In  one  of  their  earlier  experiments,  Lewin  and  Lippitt 
[25,  27]  undertook  to  study  under  relatively  controlled  conditions 
the  effects  of  democratic  and  autocratic  leadership  and  procedure 
upon  "the  unity  or  division  of  the  group  structure  pattern"  of  two 
similarly  composed  groups  of  children  performing  the  same  activ- 
ity (mask  making).  [27,  31]  An  analysis  of  extensive  records 
showed  that  the  unity  of  the  club  with  autocratic  leadership  was 
less  stable  than  that  with  democratic  leadership.  For  the  present 
discussion,  comparative  results  which  were  taken  as  indices  of 
"personal  ego  involvement  versus  group  goal  involvement"  are 
highly  relevant.  [33]  For  example,  of  the  first-person  pronouns 
used  by  the  autocratic  group,  only  18  per  cent  were  plural  (we,  us, 
ours)  as  compared  to  36  per  cent  used  by  the  democratic  group. 
After  the  groups  had  functioned  for  some  time,  members  were 
asked  to  vote  as  to  whether  or  not  the  meetings  should  be  con- 
tinued. All  of  the  autocratic-group  members  voted  to  stop,  while 
four  of  the  five  democratic-group  members  wanted  to  continue. 
At  the  same  time,  the  groups  voted  on  how  to  dispose  of  the  prod- 
ucts of  their  activities.  All  of  the  democratic-group  members 
suggested  group  disposal  of  one  or  more  of  the  masks,  while  the 
autocratic  group  members  each  claimed  his  own  work. 

Such  examples  indicate  that  members  of  the  group  more  or  less 
dominated  by  the  leader  were  less  involved  with  their  group  than 
were  members  of  the  group  in  which  co-operative  relationships 
among  leader  and  members  were  fostered.  But  the  distinction 
made  between  "ego  involvement"  and  "group  goal  involvement" 
seems  unnecessary.  When  one  participates  as  a  member  of  a 
group,  may  he  not  become  personally  involved  with  the  group,  its 
activities,  its  goals  ?  Or,  when  one  identifies  with  the  group,  may 
not  its  goals,  its  rules,  its  norms  become  his?  Lewin  and  Lippitt 
comment  in  connection  with  these  groups  of  boys:  "In  spite  of  the 
greater  spontaneity  of  expression  in  the  D-situation,  the  data  indi- 
cated that  the  A-members  were  more  frequently  observed  to  be  in 
an  overlapping  situation  where  an  individual  goal  conflicted  with 
a  group  goal  and  resulted  in  the  member  leaving  the  field  of  club 
work."  [27,  33]  It  would  seem  possible  that  one  reason  the  auto- 
cratic-group member  was  more  often  in  conflict  was  that  the  group 


REFERENCES  345 

goal  was  not  his  goal,  that  is,  he  was  less  ego-involved  than  the 
democratic-group  member.  Conversely,  one  factor  accounting  for 
"greater  spontaneity"  of  democratic-group  members  would  seem 
to  be  greater  identification  with  the  group,  that  is,  greater  ego- 
involvement. 

Without  attempting  to  summarize  the  many  interesting  and 
relevant  findings  in  this  and  other  studies  of  Lewin  and  his  stu- 
dents, their  substantiation  of  the  sociological  material  on  the  differ- 
ential behavior  of  members  toward  in-group  and  out-group  can 
be  mentioned.  In  the  study  just  cited,  "about  twice  as  many  social 
interactions,  per  units  of  interaction  possibility,  occurred  between 
in-group  members  as  between  out-group  members."  [27,  33]  In  a 
further  study  by  Lewin,  Lippitt,  and  White  [26],  fights,  reminis- 
cent of  "gang  wars,"  developed  spontaneously  between  clubs  on 
two  different  occasions.  One  fight  occurred  between  two  demo- 
cratic clubs;  the  other  occurred  between  a  democratic  and  a 
lalsscz  faire  group,  where  the  leader  exerted  little  pressure  on  the 
group. 

REFERENCES 

1.  ALLPORT,  F.  H.,  Social  Psychology,  Boston:  Houghton  Mifflin,  1924. 

2.  ANDERSON,  N.,  The  Hobo,  Chicago:  Univ.  Press,  1923. 

3.  The  juvenile  and  the  tramp,  /.  Criminal.  Law  and  Criminol.,  1923-24, 

14,  290-312. 

4. Men  on  the  Move,  Chicago:  Univ.  Press,  1940. 

5.  ASBURY,  H.,  The  Gangs  of  New  Yor^,  An  Informal  History  of  the  Underworld, 

New  York:  Knopf,  1927. 

6.  BERMAN,  N.,  The  making  of  Soviet  citizens,  Psychiatry,  /.  Biol.  and  PathoL 

Integers.  Rcls.,  1945,  8,  35-49. 

7.  BEYNON,  E.  D.,  The  Voodoo  cult  among  Negro  migrants  in  Detroit,  Am.  f. 

SocioL,  1938,  43,  894-907. 

8.  BLONDEL,  C.,  Introduction  a  la  psychologic  collective,  Paris:  Colin,  1928. 

9.  BLUMER,  H.,  Collective  behavior,  in  An  Outline  of  Principles  of  Sociology 

(R.  E.  Park,  cd.),  New  York:  Barnes  &  Noble,  1939,  221-80. 

10.  BRONFENBRENNER,  U.,  The  Measurement  of  Sociometric  Status,  Structure  and 

Development,  Sociometry  Monogr.,  no.  6,  New  York:  Beacon  House,  1945. 

11.  CANTRIL,  H.,  The  Psychology  of  Social  Movements,  New  York:  Wiley,  1941. 

12.  CHILD,  I.  L.,  Italian  or  American?    The  Second  Generation  in  Conflict,  New 

Haven:  Yale  Univ.  Press,  1943. 

13.  CLEMMER,  D.,  The  Prison  Community,  Boston:  Christopher,  1940. 

14.  DASHIELL,  F.,  Experimental  studies  of  the  influence  of  social  situations  on  the 

behavior  of  individual  human  adults,  in  Handbook  of  Social  Psychology 
(C.  Murchison,  ed.),  Worcester:  Clark  Univ.  Press,  1935,  ch.  23. 


346  EGO  IDENTIFICATIONS  IN  GROUP  SITUATIONS 

15.  DAVIS,  K.,  The  sociology  of  parent-youth  conflict,  Am.  Social.  Rev.,  1940,  5, 

523-35. 

16.  DURKHEIM,  E.,  Les  formes  elementaires  de  la  vie  religieuse,  Paris,  1912. 

17.  FAUSET,  A.  H.,  Blac%  Gods  of  the  Metropolis,  Philadelphia:  Univ.  of  Pa.  Press, 

1944. 

18.  GIST,  N.  P.,  and  L.  A.  HALBERT,  Urban  Society,  New  York:  Crowell,  copy- 

right 1942. 

19.  HEALY,  W.,  and  A.  F.  BRONNER,  New  Light  on  Delinquency  and  Its  Treatment, 

New  Haven:  Yale  Univ.  Press,  copyright  1936. 

20.  HUGHES,  E.  C,  Dilemmas  and  contradictions  of  status,  Am.  J.  Sociol.,  1945,  50, 

353-9. 

21.  LANDESCO,  J.,  Organized  Crime  in  Chicago,  in  The  Illinois  Crime  Survey, 

Illinois  Assoc.  for  Criminal  Justice,  Chicago:  Blakely,  1929,  part  3,  815-1090. 

22. The  woman  and  the  underworld,  /.  Crim.  Law  and  CriminoL,  1936,  26, 

891-902. 

23.  LA  PIERE,  R.  T.,  Collective  Behavior,  New  York:  McGraw-Hill,  1938. 

24.  LE  BON,  G.,  The  Crowd,  A  Study  of  the  Popular  Mind,  London:  Ernest  Been 

Ltd.,  1938. 

25.  LEWIN,  K.,  Field  theory  and  experiment  in  social  psychology:  concepts  and 

methods,  Am.  J.  Sociol.,  1939,  44,  6,  868-96. 

26. R.  LIPPITT,  and  R.  K.  WHITE,  Patterns  of  aggressive  behavior  in  experi- 
mentally created  "social  climates,"  /.  Soc.  Psychol,  1939,  10,  271-300. 

27.  LIPPITT,  R.,  Field  theory  and  experiment  in  social  psychology:  autocratic  and 

democratic  group  atmospheres,  Am.  /.  Sociol.,  1939,  45,  1,  26-49. 

28.  LYND,  R.,  Knowledge  for  What?    The  Place  of  Social  Science  in  American 

Culture,  Princeton:  Univ.  Press,  1939. 

29.  MARTIN,  E.  D.,  The  Behavior  of  Crowds,  New  York:  Harper,  1920. 

30.  MARTIN,  J.  B.,  A  new  attack  on  delinquency,  Harper's  Magazine,  May  1944. 

31.  MAULDIN,  B.,  Up  Front,  New  York:  Henry  Holt,  copyright  1945. 

32.  MOEDE,  W.,  Experimented  Massenpsychologie,  Leipzig:  Hirzel,  1920. 

33.  MORENO,  J.  L.,  Group  Method  and  Group  Psychotherapy,  Sociometry  Monogr., 

no.  5,  New  York:  Beacon  House,  1931. 

34.  The  Words  of  the  Father,  New  York:  Beacon  House,  1941. 

35. Who  Shall  Survive?,  Nervous  and  Mental  Disease  Monogr.,  Series  No.  58, 

Nervous  and  Mental  Disease  Publishing  Co.,  Washington,  D.  C.,  1934. 

36.  Sociometry  and  the  Cultural  Order,  Sociornetry  Monogr.,  no.  2,  New 

York:  Beacon  House,  1943. 

37.  and  HELEN  M.  JENNINGS,  Sociometric  Measurement  of  Social  Configura- 
tions Based  on  Deviation  from  Chance,  Sociometry  Monogr.,  no.  3,  New 
York:  Beacon  House,  1945. 

38.  MURPHY,  G.,  and  L.  B.  MURPHY,  Experimental  Social  Psychology,  New  York: 

Harper,  1931. 

39.  MURPHY,  G.,  L.  B.  MURPHY,  and  T.  M.  NEWCOMB,  Experimental  Social  Psy- 

chology, New  York:  Harper,  1937. 

40.  NEWSTETTER,  W.  L,  M.  J.  FELDSTEIN,  and  T.  M.  NEWCOMB,  Group  Adjustment: 

A  Study  in  Experimental  Sociology,  School  of  Applied  Social  Sciences,  West- 
ern Reserve  Univ.,  Cleveland,  Ohio,  1938. 

41.  OUTLAND,  G.  E.,  Boy  Transiency  in  America,  Santa  Barbara,  Calif.:  State  Col- 

lege Press,  1939. 


REFERENCES  347 

42.  PARK,  R.  E.,  et  al.,  The  City,  Chicago:  Univ.  Press,  1925. 

43. and  H.  A.  MILLER,  Old  World  Traits  Transplanted,  Chicago:  Univ. 

Press,  1925. 
44. Race  relations  and  certain  frontiers,  in  Race  and  Culture  Contacts  (E.  G. 

Reuter,  ed.),  New  York:  McGraw-Hill,  1934. 

45.  PIAGET,  J.,  The  Moral  Judgment  of  the  Child,  London:  Kegan  Paul,  Trench, 

Trubner  and  Co.,  Ltd.,  copyright  1932. 

46.  PUFFER,  J.,  The  Boy  and  His  Gang,  Boston:  Houghton  Mifflin,  1912. 

47.  Ross,  E.  A.,  Social  Psychology,  New  York:  Macmillan,  1908. 

48.  SAPIR,  E.,  Group,  in  Encyclopedia  of  the  Social  Sciences,  New  York:  Macmillan, 

copyright  1932,  7,  178-82. 

49.  SHAW,  C.  R.,  Delinquency  Areas,  Chicago:  Univ.  Press,  copyright  1929. 
50. The  Jac{-Roller,  Chicago:  Univ.  Press,  copyright  1930. 

51. The  Natural  History  of  a  Delinquent  Career,  Chicago:  Univ.  Press, 

copyright  1931. 

52. (ed.),  Brothers  in  Crime,  Chicago:  Univ.  Press,  copyright  1938. 

53. and  H.  D.  McKAY,  Juvenile  Delinquency  and  Urban  Areas,  Chicago: 

Univ.  Press,  copyright  1942. 
54. Memorandum  Submitted  to  the  Board  of  Directors  of  the  Chicago  Area 

Project  January  10, 1944,  mimeographed  report. 
55.  SHERIF,  M.,  A  study  of  some  social  factors  in  perception,  Arch.  Psychol.,  1935, 

no.  187. 
56. The  Psychology  of  Social  Norms,  New  York:  Harper,  1936. 

57.  SHOHLE,  R.,  The  Isolated  Religious  Sect,  master's  thesis  on  file  Univ.  Chicago 

Library,  1923. 

58.  STONEQUIST,  E.  V.,  The  Marginal  Man,  New  York:  Scribner's,  1937. 

59.  TANNENBAUM,  R,  Crime  and  the  Community,  New  York:  Ginn,  1938. 

60.  THRASHER,  F.  M.,  The  Gang,  Chicago:  Univ.  Press,  copyright  1927. 

61.  WHYTE,  W.  F.,  Corner  boys:  a  study  of  clique  behavior,  Am.  J.  Sociol,,  1941, 

46,  647-64. 

62. Street  Corner  Society,  Chicago:  Univ.  Press,  copyright  1943. 

63.  ZORBAUGH,  H.  W.,  The  Gold  Coast  and  the  Slum,  Chicago:  Univ.  Press,  copy- 
right 1929. 


CHAPTER  11 

EGO-INVOLVEMENTS  IN  CONCRETE  SOCIAL 
SITUATIONS 

In  our  review  of  experiments  and  controlled  investigations 
(ch.  6),  we  saw  how  various  kinds  of  ego-involvements  entered 
in  to  shape  or  modify  experience  and  behavior.  We  saw  how  an 
individual  identified  himself  with  certain  occupational  or  status 
groups,  how  his  role  as  a  member  of  a  class  was  related  to  affec- 
tively toned  attitudes.  We  found  that  in  some  instances  experi- 
ence and  behavior  were  modified  by  ego-involvements  which 
resulted  from  the  acceptance  of  established  norms  and  values, 
whereas  some  ego-involvements  resulted  from  the  momentary 
demands  of  the  actual  experimental  situation  in  which  the  indi- 
vidual found  himself. 

The  concept  of  ego-Involvement  is,  of  course,  not  a  mere  artifact 
created  to  account  for  artificial  laboratory  situations  or  for  the  facts 
obtained  in  other  controlled  investigations.  Indeed,  the  laboratory 
experiments  and  other  investigations  came  sometime  after  obser- 
vations of  concrete  social  situations  had  suggested  the  usefulness 
and  validity  of  the  concept.  [4, 5, 41]  Equipped  with  a  knowledge 
of  how  the  ego  develops  in  the  child,  how  it  is  re-formed  in  ado- 
lescence, how  it  is  constituted  and  how  it  affects  behavior  in  con- 
trolled laboratory  situations,  we  now  have  some  solid  basis  from 
which  to  view  ego-involved  activities  as  they  can  be  seen  in  con- 
crete social  situations. 

We  can  consider  only  a  scattered  few  of  the  thousands  of  ex- 
amples one  might  choose  for  analysis.  We  shall  proceed  from 
relatively  simple  to  more  complicated  situations.  We  repeat  again 
that  the  basic  psychological  principles  are  the  same,  whether  they 
are  demonstrated  in  laboratory  experiments,  controlled  investiga- 
tions, simple  or  complicated  situations  of  everyday  life. 

"The  apparel  oft  proclaims  the  man!9   Whatever  the  origin  or 

348 


EGO-INVOLVEMENTS  IN  CONCRETE  SOCIAL  SITUATIONS  349 

origins  of  clothes,  whatever  purposes  they  serve  in  different  cli- 
mates, there  is  little  doubt  that  one  of  their  chief  functions  is  to 
extend  the  "self  of  the  wearer,  to  enhance  his  ego,  to  display  his 
status.  Whether  it  is  the  simple  necklace  of  the  Samoan,  the  girdle 
of  the  Javanese,  the  earrings  of  the  Siberian  Eskimo,  the  tweed 
coat  of  the  American  college  man,  or  the  latest  lounging  robe 
exhibited  at  a  fashion  show  in  contemporary  Russia,  men  and 
women  reveal  the  fact  that  they  are  concerned  about  the  clothes 
they  wear,  the  impression  clothes  make  on  themselves  and  others. 
This  is  true,  of  course,  only  in  a  relative  sense.  A  destitute  person, 
suffering  from  the  ravages  of  war,  depression,  or  other  circum- 
stances, will  be  eager  enough  to  get  any  clothes  that  satisfy  his 
functional  needs.  Over  a  hundred  years  ago,  Carlyle,  in  his  pro- 
test against  conventions,  Sartor  Resartus  [8],  wrote: 

Perhaps  not  once  in  a  lifetime  does  it  occur  to  your  ordinary  biped, 
of  any  country  or  generation,  be  he  gold-mantled  Prince  or  russet- 
jerkined  Peasant,  that  his  Vestments  and  his  Self  are  not  one  and 
indivisible;  that  he  is  naked,  without  vestments,  till  he  buy  or  steal 
such,  and  by  fore  thought  sew  and  button  them.  [8,  40] 

Veblen,  in  his  penetrating  Theory  of  the  Leisure  Class  [43],  points 
out  that 

No  one  finds  difficulty  in  assenting  to  the  commonplace  that  the 
greater  part  of  the  expenditure  incurred  by  all  classes  for  apparel  is 
incurred  for  the  sake  of  a  respectable  appearance*  rather  than  for  the 
protection  of  the  person.  [43,  767  /.] 

When  Hurlock  asked  people  the  question:  "Is  your  feeling  of  self- 
confidence  increased  by  being  well  and  appropriately  dressed?" 
99  per  cent  of  the  women  and  94  per  cent  of  the  men  said 
"Yes."  [19,  46] 

In  his  well-known  book,  The  Psychology  of  Clothes  [14],  Flugel 
states  that  the  great  majority  of  authorities  "unhesitatingly  re- 
garded decoration  as  the  motive  that  led,  in  the  first  place,  to  the 
adoption  of  clothing."  [17] 

The  essential  purpose  of  decoration  is  to  beautify  the  bodily  appear- 
ance, so  as  to  attract  the  admiring  glances  of  others  and  fortify  one's 
self-esteem.  [14,  20] 


350  EGO-INVOLVEMENTS  IN  CONCRETE  SOCIAL  SITUATIONS 

This  self-esteem,  ego-enhancement,  or  ego-gratification  can,  of 
course,  be  accomplished  in  a  number  of  different  ways,  as  Flugel 
indicates.  The  most  obvious  example  is  the  function  clothes  serve 
as  an  indicator  of  rank  in  military  or  religious  circles.  And  in 
ordinary  civilian  life  clothes  provide  one  of  the  most  concrete 
expressions  of  status,  especially  in  those  areas  of  life  where  status 
is  proportional  to  the  things  money  can  buy.  Veblen  showed  how 
clothes  were  used  by  members  of  the  upper  class  to  demonstrate 
conspicuous  waste  and  conspicuous  leisure.  Flugel  notes  that 

One  woman  can  seriously  hurt  another,  even  to  the  point  of  making 
a  permanent  enemy  of  her,  by  being  better  or  more  fashionably 
dressed  upon  some  significant  occasion.  As  long  as  individuality  is 
permitted,  women  struggle  with  one  another  for  wearing  the  "latest" 
or  most  costly  frocks.  The  snobbery  of  wealth  may  even  take  a 
purely  quantitative  form,  and  it  may — and  often  does — become  a  point 
of  honour  to  wear  a  different  dress  each  day  (or  several  different 
dresses  each  day,  according  to  the  varying  occasions  of  morning, 
afternoon,  and  night).  [14, 114] 

Another  way  in  which  clothes  serve  to  enhance  the  ego  is  what 
Flugel  calls  the  "extension  of  the  bodily  self."  A  person  can 
increase  his  apparent  size,  cover  up  certain  physical  defects  or  dis- 
proportions, conform  more  to  current  norms  of  beauty  by  various 
tricks  of  dressing.  Thus  we  find  today  the  padded  shoulders  for 
both  men's  and  women's  garments,  padded  or  supported  busts  and 
tight  girdles  that  give  women  more  alluring  figures.  In  extreme 
cases,  we  have  the  long  coronation  robes  of  kings  and  queens,  the 
fancy  headdress  of  the  Indian  chief,  the  top  hat  for  the  gentleman's 
evening  wear,  and  the  long  train  of  his  lady — all  of  these  serve  the 
function  of  distinguishing  the  individual  from  others. 

We  sec  ourselves  in  the  movies.  It  has  been  pointed  out  many 
times  that  the  enormous  appeal  of  motion  pictures  is  due  in  no 
small  part  to  the  many  vicarious  satisfactions  they  provide.  By 
projecting  ourselves  into  the  characters  and  situations,  we  can 
escape  momentarily  from  humdrum  lives,  worries,  and  cares,  the 
limits  imposed  upon  us  by  our  incomes,  our  lack  of  opportunity, 
our  mediocrity  in  appearance,  ability,  or  talents. 

The  extent  to  which  this  ego-involvement  occurs  has  been 
recently  demonstrated  by  studies  of  the  tastes  and  preferences  of 


EGO-INVOLVEMENTS  IN  CONCRETE  SOCIAL  SITUATIONS  351 

movie-goers.  The  upshot  of  this  research  is  that  individuals  choose 
as  their  favorite  movie  stars  people  with  whom  they  can  most 
easily  identify  themselves— persons  of  the  same  sex,  of  comparable 
age,  and  who  tend  to  be  cast  in  roles  that  represent  a  person  of 
their  income  group.  Here  is  a  brief  summary  of  the  findings, 
taken  from  a  report  (1941)  on  audience  research:  [2] 

Most  stars  do  not  gain  support  equally  from  all  groups  of  theater- 
goers, but  appeal  particularly  to  certain  segments  of  the  population. 
Naturally,  the  variation  is  greater  in  the  case  of  some  stars  than  in 
others.  But  the  star  who  is  equally  popular  with  both  sexes,  all  age- 
groups,  all  income  levels,  and  in  all  parts  of  the  country,  is  the  rare 
exception  rather  than  the  rule. 

Theatergoers  have  a  tendency  to  project  themselves  into  the  situa- 
tion portrayed  on  the  screen,  to  imagine  themselves  in  the  place  of  the 
star,  or  (perhaps  subconsciously)  to  pretend  they  are  the  star. 

Obviously,  it  is  easier  to  imagine  yourself  in  Mickey  Rooney's  shoes 
if  you  are  a  boy  of  seventeen  than  if  you  are  a  middle-aged  housewife. 
Broadly  speaking,  that  is  why  Rooney  is  overwhelmingly  more  popu- 
lar with  boys  of  his  own  age  than  with  any  other  group  of  the 
population. 

Many  other  instances  point  to  the  importance  of  self -identification 
as  a  factor  in  determining  marquee  values.  For  example,  Judy  Gar- 
land, Deanna  Durbin  and  Linda  Darnell  are  most  popular  with  girls 
of  their  own  age.  Muni  is  strongest  with  men  over  thirty-one,  as  are 
also  Lionel  Barrymore,  William  Powell  and  Edward  Arnold.  May 
Robson  is  particularly  popular  with  elderly  females.  Joan  Crawford, 
Miriam  Hopkins,  Irene  Dunne,  Claudette  Colbert  and  Norma  Shearer 
are  most  popular  with  women  over  thirty. 

Seventy-eight  per  cent  of  the  female  stars  are  more  popular  with 
women  than  with  men.  ...  By  the  same  token  60  per  cent  of  the 
male  stars  are  more  popular  with  men  than  with  women.  The  first 
thirteen  stars,  as  ranked  by  men,  are  all  male. 

This  phenomenon  of  self -identification  can  be  seen  at  work  in  other 
fields.  For  example:  The  old  theory  that  the  best  way  to  interest 
women  in  an  advertisement  is  to  show  a  picture  of  a  man,  or  the 
best  way  to  interest  a  man  is  to  show  a  picture  of  a  woman,  has  no 
real  foundation  in  fact.  The  best  way  to  attract  women  is  to  have 
women  in  the  advertisement.  The  best  way  to  attract  men  is  by 
having  men,  or  men  and  women.  [2, 14  /.] 


352  EGO-INVOLVEMENTS  IN  CONCRETE  SOCIAL  SITUATIONS 

Advertising.  The  whole  "psychology  of  advertising"  can  be 
regarded  as  an  attempt  on  the  part  of  the  advertiser  to  identify 
the  consumer's  self-interest  with  a  particular  product.  And  the 
interest  of  the  consumers  appealed  to  are  in  no  small  part  com- 
posed of  ego-involving  values.  One  of  the  most  brilliant  and 
successful  advertisers  in  the  United  States  has  said  that  "the  whole 
secret  of  advertising  is  to  get  under  a  person's  skin,"— to  get  a 
person  so  interested  in  and  involved  with  a  product  that  he  cannot 
rest  content  until  he  buys  it.  Anyone  familiar  with  the  plethora 
of  appeals  made  to  the  public  in  most  countries  where  competition 
for  business  and  profits  is  keen,  can,  on  a  moment's  reflection, 
think  of  dozens  of  examples  of  the  advertiser's  attempt  to  protect, 
sustain,  and  enhance  his  ego.  Our  involvement  with  our  bodily 
appearance  or  its  characteristics  is  demonstrated  by  the  phenome- 
nal success  of  such  advertisements  as  those  which  tell  us  to  buy 
a  certain  mouthwash  to  avoid  "halitosis"  or  a  certain  soap  to  avoid 
"body  odor."  Our  identification  with  norms  that  determine  status 
is  fully  recognized  and  exploited  by  those  who  imply  that,  if  we 
use  or  buy  their  products,  we  will  be  in  the  same  class  as  the  distin- 
guished man  or  the  aristocratic  woman  shown  in  the  illustration. 
The  manufacturer  of  a  relatively  expensive  automobile  displays  a 
picture  of  a  car  with  a  middle-aged  man  and  woman  in  it  and 
underneath  there  is  the  bold  caption,  "Here  we  are  being  envied"; 
a  hand-lotion  concern  uses  the  ad,  "Makes  working  hands  look 
like  leisure  hands." 

Other  skillful  propagandists  as  well  as  advertising  experts  are 
well  aware  of  the  various  attitudes  which  channel  into  a  person's 
ego.  Analyses  of  different  propaganda  campaigns  in  terms  of  their 
effectiveness  in  arousing  ego-involvement  would  be  rewarding. 

What's  in  a  name?  *  Although  most  of  us  tend  to  take  our 
names  for  granted,  don't  think  much  about  them,  still,  for  nearly 
all  of  us,  there  are  occasional  instances  when  we  become  aware  of 
how  much  a  part  of  us  our  names  have  become.  We  do  not  always 
act  with  complete  neutrality  when  we  see  our  names  misspelled, 

1  This  discussion  of  names  is  largely  based  on  Robert  Holt's  Studies  in  the 
Psychology  of  Names  [18].  This  unpublished  work  is  the  most  thorough,  well- 
documented,  and  systematic  treatment  of  the  subject  that  has  come  to  our 
attention. 


EGO-INVOLVEMENTS  IN  CONCRETE  SOCIAL  SITUATIONS  353 

we  are  flattered  when  old  and  casual  acquaintances  remember 
our  names,  we  are  likely  to  feel  hurt  if  someone  we  think  should 
know  our  name  has  forgotten  it,  we  tend  to  pass  on  our  names  to 
our  children,  we  sometimes  scrawl  our  names  in  public  places  or 
on  historic  shrines.  The  vanity  of  certain  wealthy  men  is  exploited 
by  the  trustees  of  universities,  hospitals,  and  similar  institutions 
who  promise  that  the  man's  name  will  be  perpetuated  in  a  build- 
ing or  some  other  memorial  if  he  contributes  funds  to  the  institu- 
tion. The  good  salesman  and  the  good  politician  make  it  one  of 
their  first  rules  to  flatter  their  customers  and  voters  by  remember- 
ing their  names.  We  saw  in  chapter  7  how  the  small  child  learns 
that  his  name  is  a  vehicle  around  which  certain  definite  things 
happen  and  with  which  he  identifies  certain  values  and  experi- 
ences. In  interviews  with  a  great  variety  of  people,  Holt  found 
that  the  overwhelming  majority  of  them  felt  that  their  names  were 
a  part  of  them,  built  in  like  an  arm  or  a  finger,  and  not  a  fortuitous 
possession. 

Anthropologists  have  frequently  pointed  out  that  many  primitive 
men  make  no  distinction  between  the  name  and  the  person 
signified. 

Primitives  regard  their  names  as  something  concrete  and  real,  and 
frequently  sacred.  [22,  50] 

In  some  primitive  tribes  this  identification  of  the  name  and  the 
person  went  to  such  a  length  that  "to  be"  and  "to  be  named"  were 
synonymous  and  the  child  was  not  regarded  as  having  been  com- 
pletely born  until  he  was  named.  It  is  a  common  practice  among 
primitive  peoples  to  give  an  individual  a  new  name,  or  to  change 
his  name,  at  some  critical  time  of  life  such  as  puberty  or  marriage. 
Among  the  Dakota  Indians,  a  young  man  who  had  distinguished 
himself  in  battle  was  allowed  to  take  on  a  new  and  distinctive 
name;  Abram's  name  was  changed  to  Abraham,  the  Bible  tells  us, 
at  the  time  of  the  long  march  to  Palestine.  Those  familiar  with 
anthropological  literature  could  multiply  these  examples  many 
times. 

In  the  history  of  surnames  the  psychologist  finds  that  people 
who  voluntarily  chose  their  names  tended  to  identify  themselves 
with  some  positive  value  current  at  the  time.  Thus  the  name 


354  EC3O-INVOLVEMENTS  IN  CONCRETE  SOCIAL  SITUATIONS 

"John,"  almost  unknown  in  Saxon  England,  spread  widely  with 
the  return  of  the  Crusaders;  the  medieval  craftsman,  proud  of  his 
skill,  gratified  himself  with  names  such  as  Shoemaker,  Carpenter, 
Smith,  etc.;  others  took  the  names  of  high  offices  such  as  Cham- 
berlain, King,  Duke.  The  Reformation,  with  the  King  James 
version  of  the  Bible,  had  a  profound  effect  on  English  nomencla- 
ture: girls  were  named  Martha,  Mary,  Phoebe;  boys  Peter,  Paul, 
Matthew,  and  so  on.  The  strict  Puritans,  desirous  of  standing  out 
from  the  rest,  began  the  fashion  of  naming  their  children  after 
abstract  virtues.  And  so  children  got  such  first  names  as  Patience, 
Prudence,  Faith,  Hope,  etc.  This  fashion  became  so  extreme  that 
some  unfortunate  children  had  whole  phrases  as  their  first  names. 
For  example,  there  were  three  Barebone  brothers  named:  If-Christ- 
had-not-died-f or-thee-thou-hadst-been-damned  Barebone ;  Praise- 
God  Barebone;  and  Jesus-Christ-came-into-the-world-to-save  Bare- 
bone.  As  Holt  says,  in  all  of  these  instances  the  individual  was 
trying  to  "identify  the  self  with  something  with  which  there  was 
already  ego-involvement."  [18,  32] 

That  individuals  in  everyday  life  try  to  get  some  ego-enhance- 
ment from  the  prestige  they  hope  to  associate  with  their  names  is 
revealed  by  the  existence  of  various  organizations  which  will,  for 
a  fee,  send  to  those  who  write  a  complete  history  of  their  family. 
In  their  enticing  advertisements,  these  organizations  list  such 
ordinary  names  as  Jones,  Baker,  Cooper  and  Smith.  A  skeptical 
journalist  wrote  one  of  these  organizations  for  a  number  of  these 
family  accounts,  each  of  which  they  advertised  as  "separate  and 
distinct  works."  He  discovered  striking  similarities  in  them  all, 
similarities  which  built  up  the  prestige  of  the  recipient,  no  matter 
what  his  name  happened  to  be.  He  writes: 

In  each  of  25  manuscripts  there  occurs  a  paragraph  which  flatter- 
ingly summarizes  the  traits  and  characteristics  of  ancestors  in  that 
particular  family.  This  paragraph,  with  only  the  slightest  modification 
of  a  word  or  two,  reads:  "The  descendants  of  these  and  probably  of 
other  branches  of  the  family  of  America  have  spread  to  practically 
every  state  of  the  Union,  and  have  aided  as  much  in  the  growth  of 
the  country  as  their  ancestors  aided  in  the  founding  of  the  nation. 
They  have  been  noted  for  their  integrity,  industry,  energy,  courage, 
fiety,  ambition,  initiative,  resourcefulness  and  ferseverance"  Occa- 


EGO-INVOLVEMENTS  IN  CONCRETE  SOCIAL  SITUATIONS  355 

sionally,  to  add  a  nice  touch  of  distinction  to  the  family  history,  the 
order  of  the  words  was  shifted  around.  But  in  21  of  the  25  "separate 
and  distinct  works,"  these  italicized  virtues  were  identical.  [38] 

Further  confirmation  of  the  prestige  and  status  significance  of 
names  is  found  in  the  rather  common  tendency  of  persons  whose 
names  reflect  some  unpleasant  stereotype  or  negative  value  to 
change  their  names  to  something  more  common  and  accepted. 
Jews  have  had  trouble  with  their  names  all  through  their  history. 
In  various  countries  and  at  various  times,  they  have  been  forced  by 
law  or  decree  to  assume  names  that  would  distinguish  them  from 
the  non-Jews  of  a  society.  Such  laws  were  passed  in  Bohemia  in 
1787,  Napoleon  in  1808  forced  Jews  to  adopt  certain  names,  and  a 
Nazi  decree  required  "any  Jew  having  a  non-Jewish  first  name 
to  adopt  as  an  additional  given  name  Israel,  if  male,  Sarah  if 
female."  It  became  a  practice  for  officials  to  sell  names  to  Jews  so 
that  those  who  were  wealthy  were  able  to  acquire  "good"  names 
such  as  Stern  (Star),  Blum  (Flower).  A  young  man  whose  name 
had  formerly  been  Ginsberg,  but  who  had  changed  it  to  Gray, 
when  asked  why  he  had  changed  his  name  said: 

It  was  because  of  certain  associations  and  meanings  connected  with 
the  name  Ginsberg  which  did  not  apply  to  my  family.  [18,  115] 

He  commented  that  the  former  name  "suppressed  my  ego  and 
gave  me  inferiority  feelings." 

There  are  also  illustrations  of  government  edicts  commanding 
all  people  to  take  certain  types  of  names  in  the  national  interest. 
Thus  Edward  IV  of  England  in  1465  told  the  Irish  to  take  English 
surnames  or  forfeit  their  possessions;  Philip  II  of  Spain  in  1568 
forced  the  remaining  Moors  in  Spain  to  be  baptized  with  Spanish 
names.  In  a  decree  issued  in  1933,  all  Turkish  citizens  were 
ordered  to  assume  a  distinct  family  name  of  Turkish  origin.  The 
names  taken  by  many  Turks  during  this  period  reveal  their  ego 
aspirations.  Father  Divine  urges  his  "children"  to  assume  new 
names  at  the  time  they  join  his  movement,  on  the  theory  that  since 
they  have  become  new  persons  they  should  have  new  names.  [6, 
725] 

Many  people  have  so  identified  themselves  with  their  names 
that  they  have  refused  to  change  them  for  professional  reasons  or 


356  EGO-INVOLVEMENTS  IN  CONCRETE  SOCIAL  SITUATIONS 

because  of  custom.  Lucy  Stone,  one  of  the  early  agitators  in  this 
country  for  women's  rights,  refused  to  change  her  name  when  she 
was  married  and  believed  so  strongly  that  a  woman  somehow  lost 
her  identity  by  assuming  her  husband's  name  that  she  became  the 
rallying  point  for  the  Lucy  Stone  Leaguers.  As  one  of  the  Lucy 
Stoners  put  it: 

I  confess  that  I  believe  there  is  something  in  names;  that  one's 
name  is  what  Lucy  Stone  called  it  "the  symbol  of  my  identity  which 
must  not  be  lost."  The  point  of  view  is  simply  that  if  you  have  from 
your  very  first  conscious  thought  regarded  yourself  as  Anna  Maria 
Brown,  you  can't  suddenly  with  any  comfort  regard  yourself  as 
Mrs.  Thomas  Smith.  [18, 110] 

A  Southern  woman,  when  asked  by  Holt  how  she  felt  about 
changing  her  name  at  marriage,  wrote: 

To  be  strictly  truthful  in  the  matter  I  shall  have  to  admit  that  a 
strong  feeling  of  resentment  predominates.  I  remember  it  as  a  dis- 
tinct shock  when  I  was  first  called  Mrs.  Landess.  I  felt  at  first  a  sort 
of  blank  bewilderment  and  then  I  became  almost  angry — bristling 
with  resentment.  I  was  always  very  proud  of  what  Papa  made 
Hilditch  stand  for — all  my  ideals  (real  or  fancied)  I  had  cherished 
seemed  tied  up  in  that  name  and  then  to  have  to  change  it  to  one 
vastly  inferior,  which  I  came  to  realize  stood  for  nothing  but  vulgarity 
was  almost  too  humiliating.  [18,  130  f.] 

Interesting  personality  comparisons  might  be  made  of  professional 
women  who  change  their  names  when  they  are  married,  those 
who  keep  their  maiden  names,  and  those  who  adopt  the  compro- 
mise solution  of  using  both  maiden  and  married  names  together. 
The  martyr.  The  pages  of  history  are  rife  with  examples  of 
persons  who  preferred  to  die  the  death  of  a  martyr  or  a  hero  than 
to  give  up  what  to  them  were  ideals  more  important  than  life 
itself.  Giordano  Bruno,  the  Dominican  friar  of  the  sixteenth  cen- 
tury, was  condemned  to  the  flames  by  the  Inquisition  because  he 
would  not  renounce  the  ideas  of  God  and  the  universe  which  the 
Copernican  doctrine  had  inspired  in  him.  Paul  Lafargue  and  his 
wife,  daughter  of  Karl  Marx,  committed  suicide  when  they  felt 
they  had  outlived  their  effectiveness  as  protagonists  for  Marxism. 
Joan  of  Arc,  the  Maid  of  Orleans,  was  tried  as  a  heretic  and  burned 


EGO-INVOLVEMENTS  IN  CONCRETE  SOCIAL  SITUATIONS  357 

at  the  stake  because  of  her  innocent  and  unswerving  allegiance  to 
the  revelations  of  her  "voices"  which  were  not  at  all  in  conformity 
to  official  Church  interpretations.  In  such  instances  we  have 
illustrations  par  excellence  of  the  fact  that  a  value  can  and  does 
become  such  a  central  part  of  the  ego,  that  the  individual  feels 
life  is  intolerable  and  meaningless  if  this  value  cannot  be  preserved 
as  a  part  of  the  self. 

World  War  II  brought  out  in  striking  fashion — as  most  wars 
do— the  fact  that  ordinary  soldiers  and  ordinary  common  people 
sometimes  prefer  to  take  their  own  lives,  to  die  for  what  they 
believe  is  a  cause  (value)  rather  than  be  captured  by  the  enemy. 
The  utmost  heroism — self-sacrifice — was  displayed  by  men  fight- 
ing in  the  various  armies.  Particularly  dramatic  illustrations  were 
exhibited  by  certain  Japanese  soldiers  who  chose  to  blow  them- 
selves to  bits  rather  than  give  themselves  up;  suicide  (Kamikaze) 
pilots  volunteered  for  extinction;  there  were  authentic  reports  of 
individual  and  mass  suicides  of  Japanese  civilians.  These  people 
died  as  they  did,  not  because  of  any  innate  "Japanese  mentality." 
They  had  learned  thoroughly  and  felt  intensely  the  particular  set  of 
values  taught  by  a  chauvinistic  religion  which  held  that  a  life  not 
lived  in  the  service  of  the  Emperor  or  the  State  was  valueless,  while 
death  in  the  service  was  noble  and  would  be  rewarded  in  heaven. 
[37]  Heroes  in  other  armies  sacrificed  their  lives  for  other  value 
constellations. 

Ego-involvements  determined  by  situation.  We  have  already 
pointed  out  that  an  individual's  behavior  may  be  modified  or  deter- 
mined not  only  because  of  ego-involvements  due  to  the  acceptance 
of  established  norms,  but  also  because  of  more  or  less  temporary 
ego-involvement  created  by  the  demands  of  an  actual  concrete 
situation.  Thus  a  person  who  happens  to  be  at  the  scene  of  an 
accident  will  often  feel  that  it  is  his  responsibility  to  help  an  in- 
jured person  until  more  competent  professional  aid  arrives. 
When  we  take  a  long  ride  in  the  train,  get  acquainted  with  the 
other  passengers  in  our  car,  a  certain  camaraderie  often  develops, 
we  feel  that  the  people  in  that  particular  car  are  members  of  our 
group  as  distinct  from  the  passengers  in  other  cars. 

In  the  last  chapter  we  cited  an  example  of  group  formation  from 
Bill  Mauldin's  penetrating  Up  Front  [29].  At  the  very  beginning 


358  EGO-INVOLVEMENTS  IN  CONCRETE  SOCIAL  SITUATIONS 

of  the  book,  Mauldin  describes  the  identification  a  soldier  makes 
with  his  division — a  somewhat  more  permanent  identification  than 
one  that  may  be  built  up  with  a  special  outfit  assigned  to  a  tem- 
porary job  but  still  a  group  situation  brought  on  by  the  exigencies 
of  war,  not  an  established  group  such  as  the  family  or  the  church, 
characterized  by  values  that  remain  relatively  enduring. 

During  the  three  years  I  spent  in  the  45th  Division,  I  was  certain 
that  it  was  not  only  the  best  division  in  the  army,  but  that  it  was  the 
army.  Since  then  I  have  kicked  around  in  more  than  fifteen  other 
divisions,  and  I  have  found  that  the  men  in  each  of  them  are  con- 
vinced that  their  division  is  the  best  and  the  only  division.  That's 
good.  [29,  1] 

In  Men  Under  Stress,  a  book  filled  with  keen  observations, 
Grinker  and  Spiegel  [16]  describe  the  identifications  and  loyalties 
that  emerged  when  men  were  thrown  together  in  the  same  air 
combat  team  during  World  War  II. 

The  most  vital  relationship  is  not  the  purely  social.  It  is  the  feeling 
that  the  men  have  for  each  other  as  members  of  combat  teams  and 
toward  the  leaders  of  those  teams,  that  constitutes  the  essence  of  their 
relationship.  It  is  an  interesting  fact  that,  although  the  members  of 
combat  crews  arc  thrown  together  only  by  chance,  they  rapidly  be- 
come united  to  each  other  by  the  strongest  bonds  while  in  combat. 
.  .  .  The  men  and  their  plane  become  identified  with  each  other  with 
an  intensity  that  in  civil  life  is  found  only  within  the  family  circle. 
Crew  members  habitually  refer  to  each  other  as  "my  pilot"  "my 
bombardier"  "my  gunner"  and  so  on,  and  their  feeling  for  their 
plane  is  equally  strong,  since  its  strength  and  reliability  are  as  im- 
portant as  those  of  any  human  members  of  the  crew.  .  .  .  The  emo- 
tional attitudes  the  fliers  ta\e  toward  each  other  have  less  to  do  with 
the  accident  of  their  individual  personalities  than  with  the  circum- 
stances of  their  association.  .  .  .  The  men  in  the  combat  teams  are 
brothers  by  virtue  of  their  constant  enforced  association,  their  depend- 
ence upon  each  other,  their  common  ideals  and  goals,  and  their  rela- 
tion to  their  leaders.  [16,  22ft.,  italics  ours] 

In  another  book,  War  Neuroses  [17],  the  same  authors  show 
how  this  identification  with  a  group  is  one  of  the  strongest  forces 
preventing  anxiety  in  the  individual.  The  group 


EGO-INVOLVEMENTS  IN  CONCRETE  SOCIAL  SITUATIONS  359 

.  .  .  becomes  the  object  of  considerable  love  and  affection  on  the 
part  of  its  members.  They  are  proud  of  the  group,  and  resent  new- 
comers. They  are  jealous  of  other  groups,  and  strive  to  achieve  per- 
fection for  their  own.  .  .  .  Not  what  happens  to  the  individual,  but 
what  happens  to  the  group,  is  the  dominant  concern.  The  injury,  or 
even  death,  of  the  individual  is  insignificant,  compared  to  the  fate  of 
the  group.  One  pilot  with  an  anxiety  neurosis  stated  this  very  simply : 
"I  couldn't  sweat  it  out  at  the  field  while  my  crew  were  over  the 
target.  It  was  worse  than  going  along."  Through  his  identification 
with  the  group,  the  individual  shares  in  the  achievement  and  victory 
of  battle — even  if  he  should  be  injured  or  killed.  A  part  of  him  con* 
tinues  to  live  gloriously,  as  a  member  of  the  group,  no  matter  what 
his  personal  fate.  This  alteration  in  disposition  of  the  psychological 
energy  by  virtue  of  identification  with  the  group  contributes  im- 
mensely to  the  capacity  of  the  ego  to  ward  off  anxiety.  [17, 118,  italics 
ours] 

The  loss  of  morale  in  the  individual  soldier  and  the  consequent 
change  in  his  behavior  when  his  group  is  broken  up  and  he  loses 
his  established  loyalties  is  recognized  in  the  following  order  issued 
by  General  Joseph  T.  McNarney  to  all  unit  commanders  of 
American  troops  in  occupied  Germany  (April  1946):  [36,  italics 
ours] 

Due  to  rapid  demobilization  and  frequent  change  of  station  of  units 
and  assignments  of  enlisted  men  and  officers,  firm  ties  of  unit-pride 
have  been  weakened.  The  traditional  constant  concern  of  officers  for 
the  welfare  of  their  men  and  consequent  mutual  loyalties  have  been 
difficult  to  maintain  during  this  transition  period.  Team-work  often 
has  been  forgotten. 

Consequently  discipline  in  certain  localities  and  commands  in  this 
theatre  has  deteriorated  to  a  point  of  discrediting  the  fine  performance 
of  our  troops  in  general.  Indications  of  this  state  can  be  found  in : 

A.  Participation  in  black  market  activities  and  indulgence  in  drunk- 
enness. 

B.  A  high  absent-without-leave  rate  and  excessive  incidence  of 
other  disciplinary  infractions. 

C.  The  high  automobile  accident  rate. 

D.  The  excessive  venereal  disease  rate. 

E.  The  general  lack  of  smartness  in  appearance  and  conscientious 
observance  of  military  courtesy. 


360  EGO-INVOLVEMENTS  IN  CONCRETE  SOCIAL  SITUATIONS 

F.  The  complaining  attitude  toward  constituted  military  authority 
and  those  duties  essential  to  maintain  high  standards  of  soldierly 
efficiency.  [36] 

In  chapter  12,  dealing  with  the  breakdown  of  the  ego,  we  shall  see 
further  instances  of  the  effect  on  the  individual  of  the  loss  of  links 
to  other  kinds  of  groups. 

Statements  obtained  from  scientists  doing  strategic  work  on  the 
atomic  bomb  during  the  war  revealed  that  under  the  stress  of 
circumstances  they  tended  to  lose  any  interest  in  personal  acclaim 
and  identified  themselves  with  the  success  of  the  venture  as  a 
whole. 

It  was  the  newness  and  magnitude  of  the  undertaking  in  time  of 
national  crisis  which  created  a  spirit  of  cooperation  and  teamwork 
within  the  group.  One  only  had  to  note  the  lights  burning  in  the 
offices  and  laboratories  far  into  the  night  to  realize  that  time  was  of 
the  essence.  These  were  also  times  in  which  there  were  so  many 
problems  of  basic  scientific  detail  to  be  answered  that  the  young 
physicist  or  chemist,  assigned  one  question  to  answer,  felt  that  on  the 
successful  resolution  of  his  problem  depended  the  venturing  into  the 
next  stage  of  development.  .  .  .  Cloistered  theoretical  scientists,  uni- 
versity professors  and  students,  industrial  engineers,  presidents  of 
companies  large  and  small,  army  dignitaries  and  government  officials, 
all  these  were  able  to  iron  out  their  views  and  settle  differences  of 
policy  on  which  hinged  the  success  of  the  venture. 

Another  scientist  reported  that 

In  looking  back  over  my  experience  with  the  Manhattan  project,  I 
remember  a  great  deal  of  friction  and  bitter  argument,  yet  I  can  hardly 
remember  a  case  where  the  basis  of  friction  was  personal.  I  believe 
the  reason  why  the  feeling  was  so  strong  in  many  instances  was  that 
the  individuals  concerned  were  fighting  for  the  success  of  the  project 
as  a  whole  and  differing  only  about  what  course  would  lead  most 
surely  to  that  success.  The  very  obstinacy  with  which  some  main- 
N  tained  their  point  of  view  was  because  they  felt  that  if  they  gave  it 
up  they  were  giving  up  something  much  more  important  than  per- 
sonal opinions  or  prejudices.  There  were  many  instances  in  the 
project  of  men  taking  on  distasteful  jobs  merely  because  they  felt  they 
had  to  be  done.  Almost  universally  it  was  necessary  for  the  directors 
of  the  project  to  keep  the  professional  men  from  working  too  hard 


EGO-INVOLVEMENTS  IN  CONCRETE  SOCIAL  SITUATIONS  361 

rather  than  to  push  them.  I  really  believe  that  there  were  very  few 
people  on  the  project  who  were  not  willing  to  sacrifice  their  personal 
feelings  and  ambitions  for  the  over-all  success.  I  would  estimate  that 
in  the  professional  group  over  ninety  per  cent  of  the  men  that  I  know 
tried  and,  in  large  measure,  succeeded  in  merging  their  egos  with  the 
whole  work.  In  many  cases  this  attitude  extended  down  through  the 
clerical  and  technical  groups  as  well. 

By  June  of  1944,  83  per  cent  of  the  people  in  the  United  States 
felt  they  were  personally  doing  something  to  help  win  World 
War  II.  Skilled  workers,  industrialists,  housewives — as  well  as 
atomic  scientists  and  those  in  the  armed  forces— were  able  to  unite 
their  personal  values  with  a  larger  social  value.  This  identification 
was  determined  largely  by  the  circumstances  of  the  war  itself,  by 
common  threats  and  the  need  for  all-out  effort  if  all  were  to  sur- 
vive, and  not  by  any  harmony  of  established  values  that  continued 
at  war's  end.  Such  developments  as  the  wave  of  strikes  that  fol- 
lowed the  industrial  "truce"  during  the  war,  controversies  over 
racial  discrimination,  and  the  full  emergence  of  party  politics  in- 
dicated that  when  the  war  was  over  there  was  no  longer  a  unified 
clearly  understood  well-directed  "cause"  for  people  to  identify 
with.  Common  allegiances  broke  down  and  gave  way  to  separate 
often  conflicting  group  or  class  interests  which  existed  in  the  par- 
ticular social  organization. 

An  example  of  the  way  an  astute  national  leader  in  a  democ- 
racy got  people  to  take  part  in  a  decision  which  he  had  to  make 
but  which  he  knew  might  be  resented  is  found  in  President  Roose- 
velt's announcement  when  he  ordered  General  MacArthur's  escape 
from  the  Philippines.  It  was  known  from  a  public  opinion  poll, 
taken  shortly  before  MacArthur's  departure,  that  half  the  people  in 
the  United  States  felt  General  MacArthur  should  stay  with  his 
men  in  the  Philippines  to  the  bitter  end.  Roosevelt  was  therefore 
faced  with  the  possibility  that  MacArthur  would  be  branded  as  a 
deserter  by  this  sizable  number  of  Americans.  He  could  further- 
more take  it  for  granted  that  Axis  propagandists  would  capitalize 
on  the  event  to  show  that  American  generals  were  yellow  and  cow- 
ardly. The  carefully  worded  statement  issued  by  the  President 
as  the  first  announcement  of  MacArthur's  escape  not  only  soothed 
what  would  otherwise  have  been  the  ruffled  American  public  but 


362  EGO-INVOLVEMENTS  IN  CONCRETE  SOCIAL  SITUATIONS 

also  made  it  impossible  for  the  Axis  propagandists  to  make  capital 
of  the  event.  It  can  be  seen  in  the  statement  below  how  Roosevelt 
put  the  decision  up  to  every  American  citizen,  made  them  feel  it 
was  their  decision,  and  that  they  could  choose  no  other  alternative. 

I  know  that  every  man  and  woman  in  the  United  States  admires 
with  me  General  MacArthur's  determination  to  fight  to  the  finish 
with  his  men  in  the  Philippines.  But  I  also  know  that  every  man 
and  woman  is  in  agreement  that  all  important  decisions  must  be  made 
with  a  view  toward  the  successful  termination  of  the  war.  Knowing 
this,  I  am  sure  that  every  American,  if  faced  individually  with  the 
question  as  to  where  General  MacArthur  could  best  serve  his  country, 
could  come  to  only  one  answer.  [35] 

This  was  the  whole  announcement. 

The  effectiveness  of  "group  decision"  has  been  well  known 
and  practiced  for  years  in  certain  industrial  concerns,  such  as  the 
DuPont  Company  in  its  safety  programs,  the  great  TVA  develop- 
ment, and  the  recurrent  Five  Year  Plans  of  the  Soviet  Union. 
For  example,  David  E.  Lilienthal,  former  Chairman  of  the  TVA, 
in  his  penetrating  book,  TV  A— Democracy  on  the  March,  reports 
that 

From  the  outset  of  the  TVA  undertaking  it  has  been  evident  to 
me,  as  to  many  others,  that  a  valley  development  envisioned  in  its 
entirety  could  become  a  reality  if  and  only  if  the  people  of  the  region 
did  much  of  the  planning,  and  participated  in  most  of  the  decisions. 
To  a  considerable  degree  this  is  what  is  happening.  Each  year, 
almost  each  month,  one  can  see  the  participation  of  the  people,  as 
a  fundamental  practice,  grow  more  vigorous,  and,  although  it  suffers 
occasional  setbacks,  it  is  becoming  part  of  the  thinking  and  the 
mechanics  of  the  development  of  the  Tennessee  Valley.  [77,  italics 
ours] 

And  LilienthaPs  book  contains  numerous  illustrations  to  show 
how  group  decisions  were  encouraged  as  a  method  of  ensuring 
maximum  co-operation  and  effectiveness  of  effort.  A  demonstra- 
tion of  the  same  principle  is  found  later  in  a  report  by  Lewin 
of  experiments  conducted  during  the  war  in  an  attempt  to  get 
people  to  change  their  food  habits:  those  who  had  actively  helped 
in  establishing  a  group  norm  felt  the  decision  was  theirs.  As  the 


EGO-INVOLVEMENTS  IN  CONCRETE  SOCIAL  SITUATIONS  363 

report  indicates,  "the  specific  effect  of  asking  for  a  'group'  decision 
is  to  heighten  the  sense  of  involvement  and  thus  secure  greater 
participation."  [23,  9] 

Status  within  groups.  We  have  already  reported  a  number  of 
studies  which  show  the  way  in  which  an  individual  identifies  him- 
self with  a  particular  occupation,  race,  or  class,  and  the  effect  such 
identification  has  on  his  own  conception  of  status  (chs.  4  and  6). 
And  we  saw  in  the  last  chapter  how  the  individual  develops  loyal- 
ties to  particular  groups  and  the  important  role  these  loyalties  play 
in  defining  his  ego  links.  It  would,  however,  be  an  oversimplifi- 
cation of  the  real  state  of  affairs  if  any  impression  remained  that 
because  an  individual  finds  himself  placed  in  some  broad  category 
within  the  social  organization,  such  as  "skilled  worker,"  "doctor," 
"Negro,"  or  "servant,"  his  identification  with  the  group  and  the 
status  accorded  him  by  other  members  of  the  group  was  identical 
with  the  identification  and  status  of  all  other  people  who  fall 
within  the  same  general  category.  For,  on  closer  examination,  it 
will  be  found  that,  in  addition  to  the  conflicting  and  ambivalent 
loyalties  already  emphasized,  there  are  generally  very  definite 
hierarchies  and  substatus  groupings  within  any  broad  category. 
In  addition  to  his  general  status  derived  from  a  broad  reference 
group,  an  individual  acquires  a  specific  status  according  to  his 
membership  character  within  a  given  group.  Hence,  to  under- 
stand the  particular  ego  constellation  of  any  individual  in  a  pre- 
cise way,  it  becomes  necessary  to  know  how  an  individual  places 
himself  and  is  placed  by  other  members  of  a  group  within  any 
category  objectively  defined.  The  point  can  be  illustrated  with 
reference  to  concrete  observations  of  two  commonly  used  descrip- 
tive classifications— "Negroes"  and  "factory  workers." 

There  is  no  need  here  to  elaborate  the  fact  that  there  is  a  wide- 
spread stereotype  in  the  United  States  concerning  the  place  most 
white  people  feel  the  Negro  should  have  in  our  contemporary  so- 
cial organization.  The  existence  of  the  color  line  was  described  as 
follows  by  the  father  of  an  intelligent  Negro  boy  who  at  the  age 
of  14  was  just  beginning  to  be  aware  of  the  fact  that  color  made 
a  difference.  While  sitting  on  the  doorstep  of  their  home  in  a 
middle-sized  New  Jersey  city,  the  father  pointed  to  a  tramp  who 
happened  to  be  walking  along  the  street  and  said  to  his  son,  "You 


364  EGO-INVOLVEMENTS  IN  CONCRETE  SOCIAL  SITUATIONS 

see  that  tramp  standing  over  there,  Jim  ?  He's  shiftless,  common, 
and  no  good.  However,  if  he  were  to  bathe,  shave,  and  put  on  a 
clean  suit,  he  could  gain  entrance  to  any  public  place  in  the  coun- 
try. With  you  it's  different.  No  matter  what  you  do,  how  well 
educated  you  become,  you  can  never  be  treated  on  the  same  social 
level  as  that  tramp  because  of  your  color."  [28, 1] 

But  within  the  Negro  world  itself,  one  finds  approximately  the 
same  hierarchy  of  status  according  to  occupation,  education,  and 
so  on  that  exists  in  the  surrounding  white  macrocosm.  This  is 
only  one  example  of  a  minority  group  accepting  in  part  the  norms 
of  the  majority.  Negroes  who  regard  themselves  as  members  of 
the  upper  class,  while  denouncing  the  rigid  caste  barrier  between 
whites  and  Negroes  will  by  and  large  ardently  defend  a  class  struc- 
ture within  the  Negro  group  itself.  Davis,  Gardner,  and  Gardner, 
in  their  study  of  the  Deep  South  [12]  indicate  that  four  of  the 
questions  asked  by  upper  class  Negroes  of  other  Negroes  trying 
to  break  into  the  upper  class,  go  somewhat  as  follows: 

1)  What  has  been  his  education?  2)  Has  he  professional  or  semi- 
professional  status?  3)  Are  his  language,  manners,  and  dress  "pol- 
ished"? 4)  Is  he  black?  [12,2*6] 

The  answer  to  the  last  question  must  be  "No."  And  the  same 
authors  further  report  that  "the  overwhelming  majority  of  colored 
persons  are  considered  lower  class,  according  to  the  colored  group's 
own  standards!'  [222] 

A  particularly  revealing  fact  about  these  specific  status  group- 
ings among  Negroes  is  that  the  lighter  a  Negro  is  in  color,  the 
greater  chance  he  has  to  rise  in  the  social  scale.  An  unusually 
large  percentage  of  light  Negroes  is  found  in  the  upper  class  of 
the  Negro  world.  A  young  Negress  whose  very  light  skin  enabled 
her  to  pass  easily  from  the  Negro  to  the  white  world  revealed  the 
sensitivity  of  Negroes  about  their  color  when  she  reported: 

When  I  am  in  the  presence  of  dark  skinned  people,  I'm  always 
very  careful  not  to  let  the  word  "black"  enter  into  my  conversation. 
I  have  seen  dark  people  get  very  angry  when  anything  concerning 
blackness  is  brought  up.  They  seem  to  think  that  you  are  belittling 
or  making  fun  of  them.  [28,  39] 


EGO-INVOLVEMENTS  IN  CONCRETE  SOCIAL  SITUATIONS  365 

This  same  Negress,  a  college  graduate,  observed  that  many  of  the 
"better"  Negro  sororities  definitely  tended  to  exclude  girls  of  very 
dark  skins.  It  is  not  unusual  for  Negroes  when  filling  out  docu- 
ments (such  as  applications  for  a  marriage  license)  that  require 
a  designation  of  "color,"  to  be  quite  specific  if  they  are  not  them- 
selves black,  by  inserting  "light  tan,"  "brown,"  and  so  on.  Davis, 
Gardner,  and  Gardner  describe  the  way  in  which  light-skinned 
Negroes  tend  to  form  subcliques,  and  Myrdal  reports  that 

In  such  cities  as  New  Orleans,  Charleston,  Mobile,  Natchez,  and 
later  Washington,  highly  exclusive  mulatto  societies  were  formed 
which  still  exist,  to  a  certain  extent,  today.  Color  thus  became  a 
badge  of  status  and  social  distinction  among  the  Negro  people.  [32, 
696] 

The  same  study  also  reports  that 

Darker  Negroes  who  rose  from  the  masses  to  distinction  in  the 
Negro  community  by  getting  an  education  or  by  conducting  successful 
business  enterprises  showed  an  almost  universal  desire  to  marry  light- 
skinned  women  and  so  to  become  adopted  members  of  the  light- 
colored  aristocracy  and  to  give  their  children  a  heritage  of  lighter 
color.  [32,  697] 

An  experimental  demonstration  of  this  process  was  reported  in 
Marks's  study  (pp.  136/.). 

The  importance  of  skin  color  within  the  Negro  world,  together 
with  certain  practices  such  as  the  attempt  of  many  Negro  women 
to  take  the  curl  out  of  their  hair,  clearly  indicates  that  Negroes 
have  made  many  norms  of  the  white  world  their  norms,  that  by 
and  large  in  contemporary  United  States  these  norms  taken  over 
from  the  white  world  become  for  them  ego-involved  in  various 
degrees.  Because  of  his  marginal  position,  various  conflicts  in 
values  or  conflicts  of  ego  identification  are  bound  to  be  felt  by  a 
large  proportion  of  Negroes  in  our  society:  their  own  implicit 
allegiance  to  certain  bourgeois  white  standards  and  white  norms 
tend  to  run  counter  to  any  unequivocal  allegiance  to  aspirations 
certain  Negro  leaders  may  have  for  racial  solidarity  and  unity  and 
likewise  run  counter  to  class  allegiances  that  pay  no  attention  to 
culture  whether  white,  black,  or  mulatto. 


366  EGO-INVOLVEMENTS  IN  CONCRETE  SOCIAL  SITUATIONS 

The  hierarchy  of  status  among  skilled  and  semiskilled  factory 
workers  has  been  highlighted  in  the  past  decade  in  the  United 
States  by  the  intense  rivalry  between  the  two  major  labor  unions — 
one  organized  along  craft  lines,  the  other  along  industrial  lines. 
From  the  psychological  point  of  view,  it  is  by  no  means  accidental 
that  craft  unions  got  a  much  earlier  start.  For  workers,  identify- 
ing themselves  with  a  particular  job  or  skill,  could  with  relative 
ease  join  others  in  the  same  specific  occupation  to  form  an  or- 
ganized pressure  group  in  which  all  would  benefit  by  the  achieve- 
ment of  certain  goals.  The  resistance  met  by  the  Congress  of  In- 
dustrial Organizations  in  breaking  down  these  relatively  restricted 
ego-involvements  and  transforming  them  into  identifications  with 
persons  of  other  skills  in  the  same  industry  is  well  known. 

A  good  description  of  the  specific-status  hierarchy  felt  by  work- 
ers because  of  their  membership  character  in  the  same  industry  is 
given  in  the  detailed  study  of  Roethlisberger  and  Dickson:  [39] 

In  the  factory,  as  in  any  social  milieu,  a  process  of  social  evaluation 
is  constantly  at  work.  From  this  process  distinctions  of  "good"  and 
"bad,"  "inferior"  and  "superior,"  arise.  This  process  of  evaluation  is 
carried  on  with  simple  and  ready  generalizations  by  means  of  which 
values  become  attached  to  individuals  and  to  groups  performing  cer- 
tain tasks  and  operations.  It  assigns  to  a  group  of  individuals  per- 
forming such  and  such  a  task  a  particular  rank  in  the  established 
prestige  scale.  Each  work  group  becomes  a  carrier  of  social  values^ 
In  industry  with  its  extreme  diversity  of  occupations  there  are  a 
number  of  such  groupings.  Any  noticeable  similarity  or  difference, 
not  only  in  occupation  but  also  in  age,  sex,  and  nationality,  can  serve 
as  a  basis  of  social  classification,  as,  for  example,  "married  women," 
the  "old-timer,"  the  "white-collared"  or  clerical  worker,  the  "foreign 
element."  Each  of  these  groups,  too,  has  its  own  value  system.  .  .  . 
Just  as  each  employee  has  a  particular  physical  location,  so  he  has  a 
particular  social  place  in  the  total  social  organization.  ...  It  is  obvi- 
ous that  these  scales  of  value  are  never  completely  accepted  by  all  the 
groups  in  the  social  environment.  The  shop  worker  does  not  quite  see 
why  the  office  worker,  for  example,  should  have  shorter  hours  of  work 
than  he  has.  Or  the  newcomer,  whose  efficiency  on  a  particular  job 
is  about  the  same,  but  whose  hourly  rate  is  less  than  that  of  some 
old-timer,  wonders  why  service  should  count  so  much.  The  manage- 
ment group,  in  turn,  from  the  security  of  its  social  elevation,  does  not 


EGO-INVOLVEMENTS  IN  CONCRETE  SOCIAL  SITUATIONS  367 

often  understand  what  "all  the  fuss  is  about."  As  was  indicated  by 
many  of  the  studies,  any  person  who  has  achieved  a  certain  rank  in 
the  prestige  scale,  regards  anything  real  or  imaginary  which  tends  to 
alter  his  status  adversely  as  something  unfair  or  unjust.  It  is  apparent 
that  any  move  on  the  part  of  the  management  may  alter  the  existing 
social  equilibrium  to  which  the  employee  has  grown  accustomed  and 
by  means  of  which  his  status  is  defined.  Immediately  this  disruption 
will  be  expressed  in  sentiments  of  resistance  to  the  real  or  imagined 
alterations  in  the  social  equilibrium.  [39,  555  /.] 

And  the  same  authors  report  that  dissatisfaction  with  wages  is 
due  not  only  to  the  absolute  amount  of  wages  but  to  what  the 
worker  may  feel  is  an  unfair  wage  differential  between  himself 
and  someone  doing  a  slightly  different  job. 

Complaints  arise  when  wage  differentials  do  not  express  appropri- 
ately the  differences  in  social  significance  which  the  different  jobs 
have  to  the  employees  themselves.  Many  workers  who  expressed  a 
grievance  about  wages  went  on  to  say  that  the  reason  for  their  com- 
plaint was  not  that  they  were  dissatisfied  with  their  own  wages  but 
that  "it  isn't  fair."  [39,  576] 

This  is  one  more  example  of  the  fact  that  judgment  is  based  on  a 
referential  scale. 

In  addition  to  the  mixed  loyalties  caused  by  differences  in  the 
nature  of  the  jobs  themselves,  unionization,  especially  in  the  earlier 
days  when  floods  of  immigrants  were  coming  into  the  United 
States,  was  made  difficult  because  of  the  mixed  national  loyalties 
of  people  who  became  factory  workers.  MacDonald,  in  her  Labor 
Problems  and  the  American  Scene  [27],  writes  that 

The  foreign  composition  of  the  labor  force  has  had  a  profound 
effect  on  the  trade  unions.  The  difference  in  experience,  language, 
and  especially  skill  has  made  a  breach  in  the  ranks  of  organized  labor 
by  creating  an  aristocratic  group  with  little  consideration  for  the  un- 
skilled. The  division  line  between  skilled  and  unskilled  has  often 
coincided  with  that  of  native  and  alien.  ...  By  mixing  nationalities 
to  prevent  communication,  pitting  one  group  against  another,  playing 
up  the  racial  and  religious  antipathies  of  the  Old  World,  introducing 
immigrant  strikebreakers,  employers  for  a  number  of  years  were  able 
to  control  their  labor  forces  for  their  own  ends.  Constant  introduction 
of  new  recruits  tended  to  keep  the  labor  market  in  a  fluid  condition. 


368  EGO-INVOLVEMENTS  IN  CONCRETE  SOCIAL  SITUATIONS 

No  sooner  had  a  labor  group  settled  down  and  the  union  movement 
made  an  attempt  to  enlist  the  workers  than  another  immigrant  group 
was  brought  in.  [27,  237  f.] 

Job  satisfaction.  With  increasing  technological  advancement, 
the  dependence  of  more  and  more  people  on  large-scale  industrial 
production  and  distribution  both  for  a  livelihood  and  for  consumer 
goods,  one  of  the  most  crucial  problems  of  contemporary  society 
is  the  attitude  toward  the  job  held  by  those  who  occupy  dependent, 
wage-earning,  or  marginal  positions.  The  fact  that  in  the  past 
few  decades  the  rapid  industrial  development  in  the  United  States 
has  enormously  changed  the  occupational  distribution  of  gainful 
workers  can  be  illustrated  by  the  following  census  figures:  there 
is  a  marked  decrease  of  agricultural  workers,  an  increase  of  work- 
ers in  jobs  concerned  with  the  production  or  distribution  of  manu- 
factured goods.  Along  with  this  shift  in  occupation  has  gone,  of 
course,  the  familiar  shift  of  the  population  from  farm  to  urban 
areas. 

PER  CENT  DISTRIBUTION  OF  GAINFUL  WORKERS  IN  CERTAIN  OCCUPATIONS: 
1870  AND  1930  [9] 

1870  1930 

Agriculture                                                      53.0  21.4 

Manufacturing  and  mechanical  industries         20.5  28.9 

Trade                                                                 6.8  12.5 

Transportation  and  communication                    4.2  7.9 

It  is  unnecessary  to  point  out  here  that  this  increasing  indus- 
trialization has  by  no  means  been  accomplished  entirely  by  har- 
monious relationships  between  management  and  workers  in  the 
United  States  or  most  other  countries.  By  and  large,  workers  have 
"had  to  fight"  for  higher  wages,  lower  hours,  and  better  working 
conditions.  A  recent  survey  (1945)  has  shown  that  less  than  one 
third  of  skilled  and  semiskilled  workers  are  satisfied  with  their 
incomes,  whereas  nearly  60  per  cent  of  those  in  the  business  and 
management  class  say  they  are  satisfied.  [10]  When  Centers  asked 
the  question  on  a  national  survey,  "Do  you  think  working  people 
are  usually  fairly  and  squarely  treated  by  their  employers,  or  that 
employers  sometimes  take  advantage  of  them?"  less  than  a  third 
of  manual  workers  felt  that  the  working  people  were  being  fairly 
treated  whereas  approximately  half  of  all  business,  white  collar, 


EGO-INVOLVEMENTS  IN  CONCRETE  SOCIAL  SITUATIONS  369 

and  professional  people  believed  workers  were  getting  what  was 
due  them.  [10] 

We  are  calling  attention  to  the  fact  that  wages  and  hours  are 
fundamental  conditions  of  work  satisfaction  to  make  it  quite  clear 
that  these  must  be  taken  as  part  and  parcel  of  any  more  general 
consideration  of  what  constitutes  work  satisfaction.  However,  in 
addition  to  the  more  important  structural  properties  of  economic 
organization,  it  is  apparent  that  "work  satisfaction"  involves  psy- 
chological factors  that  form  a  context  within  which  satisfaction 
with  wages  and  hours  is  judged. 

We  cannot  survey  here  in  any  detail  the  vast  literature  that  has 
accumulated  on  the  problem  of  incentives  in  industry.  Summariz- 
ing a  number  of  studies  on  work  satisfaction  (1939),  Watson  con- 
cludes: 

We  have  provided  psychologically  satisfactory  employment  for  a 
large  proportion  of  the  people  in  professional  and  managerial  occupa- 
tions, and  for  a  majority  of  the  middle-class  workers  in  small  towns. 
We  have  been  much  less  successful  in  the  mines,  factories,  and  un- 
skilled trades.  [45, 123] 

And  he  points  to  the  problem  that  concerns  us  here  when  he  ob- 
serves that  certain  investigations  on  the  subject  of  job  satisfaction 
have  shown  that  there  is  "most  distress  over  ego  injuries."  H.  J. 
Ruttenberg,  research  director  of  the  Steel  Workers  Organizing 
Committee,  expressed  the  workers'  point  of  view  in  answer  to  a 
question  put  to  him  at  an  industrial  conference: 

I  live  in  a  district  where  the  workers  live,  and  spend  almost  all  of 
my  time  with  them,  and  they  are  emotionally  dissatisfied,  intellectually 
dissatisfied,  and  economically  dissatisfied  .  .  .  because  of  having  to 
respond  to  technical  changes  which  they  did  not  originate,  and  in  con- 
trast to  [workers]  who  increased  production  when  they  were  consulted 
on  technical  changes.  .  .  .  One  of  the  most  fundamental  impulses  in 
a  man  in  a  shop  is  self-expression.  If  he  is  denied  that  in  determining 
his  wages,  he  feels  much  dissatisfied.  If  he  is  permitted  expression  in 
the  production  set-up,  then  he  feels  that  he  has  made  a  contribution 
to  the  whole.  [34, 133  f.] 

From  his  review  of  industrial  leadership,  McGregor  concludes  that 
"opportunities  to  participate  in  the  solution  of  problems  and  in 


370  EGO-INVOLVEMENTS  IN  CONCRETE  SOCIAL  SITUATIONS 

the  discussion  of  actions  which  may  affect  him,  the  opportunity 
to  assume  responsibility  as  he  becomes  ready  for  it"  are  two  of 
the  conditions  necessary  if  an  industrial  worker  is  to  feel  satisfied 
and  secure.  [30,  63]  When  the  British  Institute  of  Public  Opinion 
asked  the  question  (January  1946)  "Apart  from  wages  and  secur- 
ity, what  do  you  need  most  in  a  job  for  making  you  feel  contented 
whilst  at  work?"  40  per  cent  of  all  respondents  gave  answers  that 
revealed  their  desire  for  more  active  participation  in  the  total  job — 
answers  such  as  "cooperation  between  management  and  em- 
ployee," "appreciative  employer."  Most  of  the  other  answers  given 
were  very  general  ones  such  as  "good  working  conditions,"  or  con- 
cern with  specific  desires  for  shorter  hours. 

Roethlisberger  and  Dickson  conclude  from  their  careful  investi- 
gation that2 

It  is  not  possible  to  treat,  as  in  the  more  abstract  social  sciences, 
material  goods,  physical  events,  wages,  and  hours  of  work  as  things  in 
themselves,  subject  to  their  own  laws.  Instead,  they  must  be  inter- 
preted as  carriers  of  social  value.  For  the  employee  in  industry,  the 
whole  working  environment  must  be  looked  upon  as  being  permeated 
with  social  significance.  Apart  from  the  social  values  inherent  in  his 
environment  the  meaning  to  the  employee  of  certain  objects  or  events 
cannot  be  understood.  To  understand  the  meaning  of  any  employee's 
complaints  or  grievances,  it  is  necessary  to  take  account  of  his  position 
or  status  in  the  company.  This  position  is  determined  by  the  social 
organization  of  the  company:  that  system  of  practices  and  beliefs  by 
means  of  which  the  human  values  of  the  organization  are  expressed, 
and  the  symbols  around  which  they  are  organized — efficiency,  service, 
etc.  [39,  374] 

It  is  clear  beyond  any  shadow  of  doubt  that  the  satisfaction  an 
individual  has  in  his  job  can  never  be  complete  unless  he  feels  that 
the  work  he  is  doing  is  Ms  job,  unless  there  is  some  way  in  which 
he  can  participate  through  his  job  in  some  activity  that  will  bring 
him  satisfaction.  This  satisfaction  may  be  relatively  restricted  as 
in  the  case  of  a  man  who  feels  the  importance  of  his  work  in  rela- 

2  The  failure  of  most  American  industrialists  to  take  adequate  account  of  some 
of  the  conclusions  that  can  be  derived  from  these  and  other  empirical  studies  is 
pointed  out  by  Elton  Mayo  of  the  Harvard  Business  School  in  his  book,  The 
Social  Problems  of  an  Industrial  Civilization. 


EGO-INVOLVEMENTS  IN  CONCRETE  SOCIAL  SITUATIONS  371 

tion  to  others  in  his  factory  unit;  it  may  involve  the  larger  satis- 
faction he  feels  *s  an  important  cog  in  the  whole  industrial  plant; 
or  a  worker  may  identify  himself  and  his  job  with  the  fate  and 
progress  of  a  whole  social  organization  or  with  workers  in  all 
countries. 

It  is  not  our  task  here  to  discuss  the  relative  effectiveness  with 
which  this  job  identification  can  be  accomplished  under  different 
systems  of  economic  organization.  But  we  can  learn  something 
of  the  psychological  components  of  job  satisfaction  among  indus- 
trial workers  in  the  United  States  if  we  examine  a  concrete  case 
where  job  satisfaction  seems  to  be  extremely  high.  We  take  as  our 
example  the  Lincoln  Electric  Company  which  has  received  a  great 
deal  of  publicity.  Just  what  conditions  prevail  in  the  Lincoln 
Electric  Company  to  make  for  job  satisfaction  ?  Why  does  it  stand 
out  as  a  plant  where  there  appears  to  be  complete  harmony,  co- 
operation, and  mutual  confidence  between  "labor"  and  "manage- 
ment"? 

A  report  issued  by  the  United  States  Department  of  Labor  on 
incentive  wage  plans  (1942)  states  that  the  grievances  workers 
have  against  scientific  management 

...  are  attributable  to  the  tendency  of  management  engineers  to 
consider  labor  as  impersonal  and  as  a  part  of  the  machine  process. 
Workers  feel  that  they  are  being  treated  as  abstract  "labor"  rather  than 
human  beings  at  work.  [21, 7] 

The  undeniable  success  of  the  Lincoln  Electric  Company  seems 
to  be  due  basically  to  the  feeling  of  its  President  that  "labor  is  an 
individual  not  a  commodity."  [24,  203]  And  for  Mr.  Lincoln  the 
statement  was  more  than  words  handed  out  by  a  public  relations 
department  or  aired  at  a  management-labor  conference.  He  pro- 
ceeded to  try  to  create  the  conditions  within  which  the  individual 
could  participate  as  an  individual  in  the  total  job  of  the  plant. 
One  of  the  first  things  he  did  was  to  encourage  the  formation  of 
an  advisory  board  chosen  from  the  entire  personnel  of  the  plant, 
one  man  being  elected  from  each  department  by  all  members  of 
that  department  The  men  on  the  board,  together  with  the  plant 
superintendent  and  Mr.  Lincoln,  who  acts  as  chairman,  serve  as 


372  EGO-INVOLVEMENTS  IN  CONCRETE  SOCIAL  SITUATIONS 

the  board  of  directors  of  the  plant.  Here  are  a  few  of  the  deci- 
sions this  advisory  board  has  put  into  operation:  installed  a  piece- 
work incentive  plan  with  rates  guaranteed  by  the  company  and 
with  the  worker  having  the  right  to  challenge  the  rates  set;  in- 
sured all  workers  for  the  equivalent  of  a  year's  wages  without  cost 
to  the  individual  worker;  provided  two  weeks  vacation  with  pay, 
closing  the  entire  factory  for  the  purpose;  issued  stock  to  the  em- 
ployees who  want  it  (over  half  are  stockholders);  established  a 
suggestion  system  and  provided  that  anyone  whose  suggestion  for 
more  efficient  production  is  accepted  will  receive  half  of  the  net 
estimated  savings  for  the  first  year  of  use  (approximately  50  new 
suggestions  are  turned  in  every  month  and  about  one  tenth  of 
them  prove  useful);  inaugurated  a  bonus  system  whereby  each 
worker  receives  what  is  regarded  as  his  share  of  the  bonus  accord- 
ing to  his  value  to  the  company;  installed  an  annuity  plan  and  a 
trust  fund;  set  up  an  employee  association  to  provide  for  social 
and  athletic  activity  and  which  has,  for  example,  a  sick  committee 
whose  members  have  among  their  duties  the  obligation  to  visit 
within  48  hours  anyone  who  has  not  reported  to  work  because  of 
illness. 

Some  of  the  claims  which  the  Lincoln  Electric  Company  can 
make  (in  the  early  1940's)  are  the  following:  Lincoln  workers 
produce  more  per  hour  than  any  organization  making  a  com- 
parable product  in  the  world;  Lincoln  Electric  factory  workers  are 
the  highest  paid  employees  in  industry  anywhere  in  the  world. 
In  1943  the  average  annual  earnings  for  all  employees  was  $5,539. 
Approximately  90  per  cent  of  Lincoln  employees  own  their  own 
homes.  There  is  practically  no  labor  turnover:  of  260  employees 
on  the  payroll  ten  years  ago,  203  are  still  with  the  company.  Lin- 
coln selling  prices  are  less  than  those  of  any  company  making  a 
comparable  product. 

Lincoln  himself  describes  his  plan  as  one  based  on  "intelligent 
selfishness."  [25]  What  he  means  by  this  is  that  each  individual 
in  the  plant  sees  his  relationship  to  the  plant  as  a  whole  and 
realizes  that  "he  is  a  vital  part  of  one  going  whole."  The  worker 
knows  that  the  company's  success  is  his  success  and  Lincoln  em- 
ployees do  not  seem  to  share  the  fear  so  common  in  many  indus- 


EGO-INVOLVEMENTS  IN  CONCRETE  SOCIAL  SITUATIONS  373 

tries  with  incentive  wage  plans  that  they  will  either  work  them- 
selves out  of  a  job  or  that  rates  will  be  cut.  According  to  Mr. 
Lincoln,  the  plan  under  which  his  company  operates 

...  has  changed  our  workers  from  people  who  are  working  at  a 
job  at  so  much  an  hour  into  people  who  feel  their  success  is  tied 
definitely,  completely  and  proportionately  into  the  success  of  the  com- 
pany itself.  They  are  not  working  at  a  job  so  many  hours  a  day  at  so 
much  per  hour.  They  are  working  to  get  a  job  done.  This  change  of 
attitude  from  working  so  many  hours  a  day  for  so  much  money  into 
the  desire  of  working  to  produce  a  certain  result  produces  unbelievable 
results  in  efficiency  of  production.  [33,  21] 

An  employee  of  the  company  puts  it  as  follows: 

I  know  that  the  more  I  make,  the  more  my  company  is  able  to  make. 
Every  time  any  of  us  increases  our  efficiency,  the  amount  saved  is 
proportioned  between  me  and  the  rest  of  my  business  partners.  [47,  4] 

This  particular  employee  regards  himself  as  a  business  man  with 
definite  status: 

For  some  reason  or  other,  a  lot  of  people  seem  to  be  impressed 
with  the  idea  that  because  you  work  where  the  labor  of  production 
gets  your  hands  dirty,  you  have  no  right  to  be  paid  more  than  so  much 
a  year.  .  .  .  Most  of  these  fellows  work  with  their  hands  and  get  them 
pretty  dirty  sometimes,  too.  Yet  no  one  ever  questions  whether  they 
are  worth  the  incomes  they  make.  I  look  at  welding  as  a  kind  of 
professional  business,  an  art  I  have  to  be  good  at  in  order  to  keep  up 
with*  the  rest  of  the  businessmen  who  work  around  me  here  at 
Lincoln.  [47,  4} 

The  Lincoln  Electric  Company,  as  we  pointed  out,  is  unusual. 
The  particular  combination  of  factors  that  makes  it  what  it  is  also 
makes  it  something  of  a  little  world  of  its  own.  The  company's 
practices  deviated  so  much  from  the  usual  ones  in  American  in- 
dustry that  during  World  War  II  it  was  kept  busy  justifying  its 
methods  to  government  committees  and  departments.  There  is 
no  union  in  the  Lincoln  Electric  Company  and  the  employees, 
therefore,  regard  themselves  more  as  "businessmen,"  identify  them- 
selves with  the  company  and  its  welfare,  not  with  other  working 
people  in  the  larger  world  outside.  In  other  words,  employee 


374  EGO-INVOLVEMENTS  IN  CONCRETE  SOCIAL  SITUATIONS 

identifications  with  their  jobs  can  be  regarded  as  occurring  within 
a  relatively  restricted  and  isolated  industrial  microcosm:  a  micro- 
cosm with  its  own  norms  and  values.  Hence  the  Lincoln  em- 
ployee, in  identifying  himself  with  his  particular  organization, 
does  not  simultaneously  and  automatically  identify  himself  with 
more  widespread  values  common  to  the  whole  social  organization 
within  which  his  company  is  but  a  small  part.  Just  what  would 
happen  to  the  Lincoln  plan  and  just  what  identifications  Lincoln 
Electric  workers  would  make  if  there  were  a  major  depression  are 
moot  questions. 

Lilienthal's  report  of  the  development  of  the  TVA  clearly  re- 
veals the  sense  of  identification  and  participation  an  individual 
worker  can  obtain  if  conditions  are  such  that  it  is  apparent  to 
him  that  in  working  for  a  whole  regional  project  he  is  benefiting 
himself.  He  writes: 

In  pouring  concrete  so  that  the  Douglas  Dam  could  be  built  on  a 
world-record  schedule,  in  tending  the  glow  of  the  giant  electric  fur- 
naces at  Muscle  Shoals,  or  in  stringing  aluminum  and  copper  cable 
along  the  line  of  march  of  transmission  towers,  TVA  workers  know, 
and  show  they  know,  that  in  thus  working  for  their  valley  they  are 
working  for  themselves;  they  build  for  themselves.  [97] 

And  as  Lilienthal  points  out,  incentives  based  on  identifications 
with  group  norms  can  be  stronger  than  incentives  based  on  the 
"profit  motive"  alone: 

Whether  in  private  business  or  public  service  a  man's  conviction 
that  he  is  performing  an  important  service  for  others,  that  he  is 
part  of  something  far  more  important  than  himself,  is  a  measure  to 
him  of  the  importance  of  that  job.  It  is  this,  I  think,  that  accounts 
in  considerable  part  for  the  continued  enthusiasm  of  the  TVA  tech- 
nical staff  long  after  the  newness  of  the  undertaking  has  passed,  a 
spirit  that  has  been  observed  and  remarked  upon  by  a  long  succession 
of  visitors  from  other  parts  of  the  country  and  the  world.  The  notion 
is  naive  that  only  by  the  incentive  of  pay  or  profit  do  men  "keep  on 
their  toes"  and  do  their  best  work.  Many  of  the  TVA's  key  staff 
members  are  earning  less  than  in  the  posts  they  left  to  join  this 
job.  \122] 


EGO-INVOLVEMENTS  IN  CONCRETE  SOCIAL  SITUATIONS  375 

At  the  present  time,  the  identification  of  a  worker  with  his  job 
in  a  factory,  mine,  or  farm,  and  his  simultaneous  identification 
with  the  larger  values  and  purposes  of  the  whole  social  organiza- 
tion through  his  job  can  be  best  illustrated  with  reference  to  the 
role  of  the  worker  in  the  Soviet  Union.  In  a  socialist  organization 
where  all  means  of  production  and  exchange  are  owned  and  oper- 
ated by  the  state,  the  possibility  exists  for  each  worker,  no  matter 
what  his  job,  to  identify  himself  with  the  purposes  and  aspirations 
of  the  whole  society.  Socialism  and  Communism  are  constantly 
held  up  to  the  people  of  modern  Russia  by  their  leaders  as  the 
goals  toward  which  each  member  of  the  society  should  strive.  The 
principles  of  socialism  and  Communism,  as  defined  by  Stalin,  are 
as  follows: 

The  principle  of  Socialism  is  that  in  a  socialist  society  each  works 
according  to  his  ability  and  receives  articles  of  consumption,  not  ac- 
cording to  his  needs,  but  according  to  the  work  he  performs  for  soci- 
ety. This  means  that  the  cultural  and  technical  level  of  the  working 
class  is  still  not  a  high  one,  that  the  distinction  between  mental  and 
manual  labor  persists,  that  the  productivity  of  labor  is  still  not  high 
enough  to  ensure  an  abundance  of  articles  of  consumption,  and,  as  a 
result,  society  is  obliged  to  distribute  articles  of  consumption,  not  in 
accordance  with  the  needs  of  the  members  of  society,  but  in  accordance 
with  the  work  they  perform  for  society.  Communism  represents  a 
higher  stage  of  development.  The  principle  of  Communism  is  that  in 
a  Communist  society  each  works  according  to  his  abilities  and  receives 
articles  of  consumption,  not  according  to  the  work  he  performs,  but 
according  to  his  needs  as  a  culturally  developed  individual  [42,  6] 

In  other  words,  the  aspiration  is  that  the  standard  of  living  and 
the  cultural  level  of  all  workers  should  eventually  be  raised  to 
the  level  enjoyed  by  the  most  advanced  technicians  or  profession- 
als. This  is  the  ultimate  goal  which,  as  Stalin  says,  is  now  far 
from  realization. 

The  point  that  concerns  us  here  is  the  fact  that  such  a  general 
goal  exists,  is  explicitly  stated,  and  that  a  great  effort  is  made  by 
the  Government  and  party  members  to  keep  the  goal  constantly 
before  the  eyes  of  the  people.  Status  and  social  approval  are  meas- 
ured in  terms  of  the  usefulness  of  an  individual's  labor  in  achiev- 
ing the  common  goal.  Particularly  efficient  workers  are  praised 


376  EGO-INVOLVEMENTS  IN  CONCRETE  SOCIAL  SITUATIONS 

in  the  press  and  honored  with  medals.  Their  photographs  appear 
on  special  bulletin  boards  of  their  factories.  They  become  "Heroes 
of  the  Soviet  Union."  For  example,  during  World  War  II,  hun- 
dreds of  official  decorations  were  awarded  to  workers  in  the  rear 
as  well  as  to  members  of  the  fighting  forces.  Special  distinctions 
are  bestowed  on  factory  or  agricultural  units  that  show  particular 
initiative  or  efficiency.  Similarly  every  effort  is  made  to  turn  pub- 
lic opinion  against  those  who  do  not  seem  to  be  doing  their  share. 
It  is  in  this  general  context  that  the  Stakhanov  movement,  de- 
veloped in  the  middle  1930's,  assumed  its  particular  significance. 
Although  from  a  technical  point  of  view,  it  introduced  no  methods 
not  known  and  practiced  in  more  advanced  industrial  countries 
[26],  and  although  the  zest  with  which  Stakhanovism  was  taken 
up  developed  certain  weaknesses  and  exaggerations  because  of 
hasty  application  [13],  there  can  be  little  doubt  that  the  solid 
achievements  it  represented  in  the  long  run  were  largely  possible 
because  the  individual  Stakhanovite  in  trying  to  raise  his  own 
production  standards  was  not  only  increasing  his  income  but  help- 
ing to  achieve  a  national  goal  that  he  understood  and  identified 
himself  with.  In  becoming  a  Stakhanovite,  an  individual  was  be- 
coming a  national  hero.  For  example,  Molotov  told  the  All-Union 
Conference  of  Stakhanovites: 

Do  we  not  know  that  the  most  popular  representatives  of  the 
Stakhanov  movement  are  rank-and-file  working  men  and  women? 
Yesterday  they  were  still  unknown  to  many  even  in  their  own  fac- 
tories, yesterday  they  were  on  a  level  with  the  other  rank-and-file 
working  men  and  women.  But  today  the  whole  working  class  of  the 
Soviet  Union  knows  the  names  of  its  new  heroes,  the  names  of  those 
working  men  and  women  who  have  become  the  standard-bearers  of 
the  new  movement  in  the  fight  for  socialism.  [31, 4] 

Stakhanovism  was  regarded  in  the  Soviet  Union  as  a  new  form 
of  "socialist  competition."  There  was  not  only  competition  be- 
tween individual  workers  but  competition  between  factories,  fac- 
tory departments,  mines,  and  collective  farms.  This  socialist  com- 
petition, the  general  principles  of  which  were  laid  down  by  Lenin 
in  1918,  holds  the  possibility  of  leading  to  cohesion  rather  than  to 
a  division  of  interest.  For  the  competitor,  whether  he  acts  as  an 


EGO-INVOLVEMENTS  IN  CONCRETE  SOCIAL  SITUATIONS  377 

individual  or  as  a  member  of  a  larger  unit,  competes  within  the 
larger  framework  of  a  common  goal  which  is  constantly  made 
clear  through  the  press,  over  the  radio,  and  in  organizational 
meetings.  "The  resultant  change  in  the  psychological  atmos- 
phere," as  Ward  observed  it,  "is  one  of  the  things  that  causes 
the  visitor  to  realize  that  he  is  in  a  new  world."  [44,  133] 

In  a  speech  to  the  members  of  the  First  Conference  of  Stak- 
hanovites  held  in  the  Kremlin  in  1935,  Stalin  told  them: 

People  in  our  country  do  not  work  for  exploiters,  for  the  enrichment 
of  parasites,  but  for  themselves,  for  their  own  class,  for  their  own, 
Soviet  society,  where  government  is  wielded  by  the  best  members  of 
the  working  class.  That  is  why  labor  in  our  country  has  social  sig- 
nificance and  is  a  matter  of  honor  and  glory.  .  .  .  Here  the  man  who 
labors  is  held  in  esteem.  Here  he  works  not  for  the  exploiters,  but  for 
himself,  for  his  class,  for  society.  Here  the  man  who  labors  cannot 
feel  neglected  and  solitary.  .  .  .  And  if  he  works  well  and  gives  society 
all  he  can — he  is  a  hero  of  labor  and  is  covered  with  glory.  [42, 13] 

This  identification  of  the  Soviet  worker  with  the  Communist 
goal  is  seen  in  reports  revealing  the  conviction  of  the  Soviet  citi- 
zens that  what  the  state  accomplishes,  he  accomplishes;  that  what 
the  state  owns,  he  owns;  that  his  standard  of  living  will  be  raised 
by  collective  effort.  An  American  observer  writes  that  "The  Soviet 
people  have  learned  from  experience  that  'what's  ours  is  mine.' " 
[44, 40]  A  system  of  ownership  has  been  developed  "which  makes 
it  true  that  when  a  man  works  for  others  he  is  also  working  for 
himself."  [43  f.]  A  British  engineer  working  in  Russia  has  said 
that 

For  an  engineer,  a  maker  of  machines,  work  in  a  Soviet  factory 
offers  tremendous  satisfaction.  The  commercial  principle  that  holds 
sway  in  capitalist  industry  very  often  forces  engineers  to  spend  their 
energy,  strength,  and  knowledge  for  nothing,  several  factories  turning 
out  one  and  the  same  article,  each  striving  when  an  order  comes  to 
secure  the  contract.  All  making  new  designs  but  only  one  obtaining 
the  order.  The  other  designs  are  wasted.  In  the  Soviet  Union,  on 
the  other  hand,  all  tasks  are  linked  up  with  the  development  of  the 
industry  and  the  engineer  knows  that  the  plan  he  has  drawn  up  will 
be  used.  [20,  24] 


378  EGO-INVOLVEMENTS  IN  CONCRETE  SOCIAL  SITUATIONS     \ 

The  conversations  Pearl  Buck  reported  with  a  Russian-born  and 
educated  girl  are  revealing.  This  young  woman,  who  had  come 
to  the  United  States  with  her  husband,  was  not  a  Communist. 
Buck  reports: 

I  discovered  after  our  first  conversation  that  she  was  even  rather 
vague  about  the  political  theories  of  communism.  She  judged  her 
country  solely  by  pragmatic  tests.  What  was  good  she  knew  because 
it  had  been  good  for  her  and  her  family.  What  was  bad  was  bad 
because  it  had  not  lived  up  to  her  expectations.  [3, 10] 

Here  are  a  few  excerpts  from  the  conversations. 

"What  did  the  village  do  with  the  kulak's  brick  factory?"  I  asked. 

"It  belonged  to  the  collective  farm,"  Masha  said.  "Those  who 
wanted  to  work  there  instead  of  in  the  fields  could  do  so.  The  people 
now  felt  the  bric\  factory  was  theirs,  and  they  worked  harder  than 
ever  and  the  production  was  higher  than  before.  I  worked  in  this 
brick  factory  in  the  summer  for  several  seasons,  and  of  course,  if  you 
are  interested  in  profits,  you  compete,  and  we  actually  did  two  days' 
work  in  one.  Whoever  produced  the  most  was  respected  by  others. 
It  was  interesting  and  good  work,  and  our  collective  farm  took  the 
profits  and  in  this  way  we  built  new  cow  barns,  and  we  bought  new 
machines  for  the  fields  and  to  work  on  the  flax."  [49,  italics  ours] 

"But  I  want  to  tell  you  about  the  evening  school.  Those  people 
worked  all  day  and  they  came  directly  from  the  factory  to  the  school, 
which  was  in  a  barracks.  During  the  little  free  time  before  school 
began,  they  read  and  studied.  Sometimes  they  discussed  the  day's 
work,  and  I  remember  they  spoke  especially  about  quotas  of  work, 
whether  this  shop  or  that  factory  had  lived  up  to  its  program.  They 
felt  the  responsibility  for  the  worJ^.  .  .  ."  [3,  94,  italics  ours] 

A  Stakhanovite  girl  who  had  broken  many  production  records 
in  the  textile  mill  said  to  an  American  observer: 

We  work  in  our  own  mill,  we  work  for  ourselves,  without  any 
bosses,  for  our  country  .  .  ,  and  that  is  why  we  are  able  to  tend  such 
a  large  number  of  looms.  [15, 13  f*] 

This  possible  identification  of  the  Soviet  worker  with  the  values 
of  the  whole  social  organization  has  been  very  neatly  stated  by 
the  Soviet  psychologist  Rubinstein: 


EGO-INVOLVEMENTS  IN  CONCRETE  SOCIAL  SITUATIONS  379 

The  very  fact  of  social  life  and  social  division  of  labor  naturally,  by 
an  internal  necessity,  brings  it  about  that  the  activity  of  man  is  directly 
aimed  at  the  satisfaction  not  merely  of  his  own  personal  needs  but  of 
those  of  society  as  well.  To  satisfy  his  own  needs,  a  man  must  make 
the  satisfaction  of  social  needs  the  direct  goal  of  his  actions.  .  .  .  The 
socially  important,  becoming  the  personally  important  while  still  re- 
maining the  socially  important,  arouses  in  the  individual  tendencies 
and  forces  of  great  strength  .  .  .  the  socially  important,  becoming 
personally  important  for  the  individual,  arouses  in  him  forces  much 
more  powerful  than  those  evoked  by  personal  desires  alone,  and  differ- 
ent from  them  in  their  content,  origin  and  significance.  [40,  795,  italics 
ours] 

Special  attention  should  be  drawn  to  the  last  part  of  Rubinstein's 
statement.  For  he  brings  out  here  a  fact  we  have  repeatedly  em- 
phasized, namely,  that  in  new  social  situations  new  norms  and 
values  often  arise,  that  the  individual  identifies  himself  with  these 
emerging  values  and  exhibits  a  behavior  which  qualitatively  as 
well  as  quantitatively  can  and  often  does  differ  from  the  behavior 
"characteristic"  of  him— "characteristic"  only  because  of  the  par- 
ticular constellation  of  social  determinants  in  his  more  usual  en- 
vironment, and  which  can  change  radically  when  those  deter- 
minants are  altered.8 

In  this  particular  publication,  Rubinstein  gives  no  psychological 
underpinning  for  his  observation  that  what  is  socially  important 
can  be  personally  important  and  vice  versa.  And  he  himself  poses 
as  "one  of  the  greatest  tasks  of  psychology"  the  study  of  "how 
these  moral  motives  arise  and  operate,  how  the  individual  rises 
above  the  merely  personal  to  the  socially  important,  and  how  the 
socially  important  becomes  personally  important  for  him."  [795] 
It  seems  to  us  that  the  answer  to  this  question  is  found  in  the 
genetic  formation  of  the  ego,  the  incorporation  of  social  values  into 
the  ego  structure  of  the  individual,  the  identification  of  the  self 
with  established  values  of  a  group  or  with  new  norms  that  may 
arise  in  new  social  situations.  There  will  be  "harmony,"  "integra- 

8  We  repeat  again  that  we  are  not  ignoring  here  individual  differences  in  tem- 
perament and  ability  and  certain  characteristic  tendencies  of  behavior  which  they 
determine.  But  the  relationship  of  these  personality  characteristics  and  traits  to 
the  social  environment  and  to  particular  constellations  of  situational  determinants 
cannot  be  analyzed  in  detail  here. 


380  EGO-INVOLVEMENTS  IN  CONCRETE  SOCIAL  SITUATIONS 

tion,"  "adjustment"  between  the  individual  ego  and  the  values  of 
various  groups  or  the  common  values  of  a  whole  social  organiza- 
tion insofar  as  the  established  values  of  groups  or  social  organiza- 
tions are  objectively  compatible  with  one  another. 

In  a  society  where  the  values  of  different  class,  status,  occupa- 
tion, religious,  racial,  or  nationality  groups  conflict,  in  which  di- 
lemmas are  produced  by  conflicting  statuses,  an  individual  almost 
invariably  finds  himself  placed  within  two  or  more  of  these  con- 
flicting groups.  He  will  almost  inevitably  be  torn  in  his  loyalty, 
ego-involved  to  a  greater  or  lesser  extent  with  values  that  he  can- 
not make  jibe  with  each  other.  Hence  he  will  feel  subjectively 
torn,  frustrated,  maladjusted.  This  conflict  of  loyalties  was  clearly 
indicated,  for  example,  in  a  study  of  the  attitudes  of  U.  S.  coal 
miners  made  by  the  Office  of  Public  Opinion  Research  in  early 
May  1943.  [7]  At  that  time  the  miners'  leader,  John  L.  Lewis, 
was  threatening  to  call  a  strike.  A  temporary  truce  had  been  ar- 
ranged while  negotiations  continued.  The  war  was  raging,  and, 
if  a  strike  were  called,  government  seizure  of  the  mines  seemed 
inevitable.  The  workers  were  asked  if  they  would  favor  or  oppose 
calling  a  strike.  The  representative  sample  voted  two  to  one 
against  a  strike.  They  gave  as  their  chief  reason  their  desire  not 
to  let  down  the  war  effort,  their  confidence  in  President  Roosevelt 
as  a  man  who  would  give  them  a  square  deal.  They  preferred, 
almost  two  to  one,  to  follow  Roosevelt's  orders  as  Commander- 
in-Chief  to  orders  of  their  union  leaders.  At  the  same  time,  how- 
ever, over  three  fourths  of  them  said  that,  if  a  strike  were  called, 
whether  they  wanted  it  or  not,  they  would  go  out— they  couldn't 
let  their  union  and  their  union  leaders  down.  As  Roethlisberger 
and  Dickson  point  out  in  their  impressive  studies  of  workers  in 
an  American  industry,  there  is  in  the  United  States  often  consid- 
erable conflict  between  an  individual's  personal  values  and  the 
values  accepted  by  him  and  his  immediate  coworkers,  the  values 
held  by  management,  or  the  values  the  worker  encounters  outside 
the  factory  when  he  assumes  the  role  of  a  citizen.  They  conclude: 

Where  the  social  conditions  of  work  are  such  as  to  make  it  difficult 
for  a  person  to  identify  himself  or  his  task  with  any  social  function, 
the  worker  is  also  liable  to  obsessive  responses  and  hence  to  a  dimin- 
ished capacity  for  work.  [39,  328] 


EGO-INVOLVEMENTS  IN  CONCRETE  SOCIAL  SITUATIONS  381 

And  they  state  further  that  the  ultimate  significance  of  an  indi- 
vidual's work 

...  is  not  defined  so  much  by  his  relation  to  the  company  as  by 
his  relation  to  the  wider  social  reality.  Only  in  terms  of  this  latter 
relation  can  the  different  attitudes  of  satisfaction  or  dissatisfaction  of 
individuals  who  are  presumably  enjoying  the  same  working  environ- 
ment and  occupational  status  be  understood.  [39,  376] 

In  a  revealing  study  on  some  of  the  factors  that  make  for  work 
satisfaction,  W.  T.  Watson  concluded  from  his  analysis  of  the  case 
histories  of  a  variety  of  workers  that  "some  form  of  restlessness  or 
mental  dissatisfaction  seems  always  to  be  present"  if  workers  are 
separated  "in  spirit  and  objective,"  if  they  cannot  see  some  general 
meaning  or  value  in  their  job.  [46,  255]  All  these  results  confirm 
Cooky's  earlier  observations  concerning  the  importance  of  the 
various  roles  a  worker  plays  in  society  in  addition  to  his  role  as 
a  producer.  [11] 

On  the  other  hand,  the  worker  in  the  Soviet  system  has  the  pos- 
sibility of  finding  an  extension  of  the  values  held  by  his  local  fac- 
tory unit  to  the  whole  factory  or  the  collective  operation  and,  in 
turn,  to  the  larger  goal  of  the  whole  social  organization.  Thus 
the  values  which  are  for  him  personal  values  and  which  give  him 
status  in  his  own  eyes  and  the  eyes  of  his  fellow  workers  are  part 
and  parcel  of  or  reflections  of  values  of  larger  social  organizations 
of  which  he  and  his  immediate  group  regard  themselves  a  part. 

The  upshot  of  all  this  is,  then,  that  the  individual  will  feel  con- 
sistently  and  harmoniously  ego-involved  in  proportion  to  the  ob- 
jective consistency  and  harmony  of  the  many  social  situations  and 
values  which  form  his  particular  ego  constellation.  Without  un- 
derstanding the  relationship  of  the  values  which  constitute  the  ego, 
the  extent  of  conflict  between  or  overlapping  of  those  values,  we 
cannot  understand  either  the  dynamics  of  industrial  morale  or  the 
reasons  for  the  differential  participation  of  a  given  individual  in 
his  job  and  the  gratification  that  job  provides  him.  The  "active" 
and  "reactive"  egos  of  industrial  workers  described  by  G.  W.  All- 
port  [1]  become  meaningful  when  there  is  an  understanding  of 
the  relationship  between  values  that  have  become  personal  and 
value  judgments  encountered  in  actual  working  conditions. 


382  EGO-INVOLVEMENTS  IN  CONCRETE  SOCIAL  SITUATIONS 

This  conflict  between  the  values  of  small  social  units  or  between 
the  values  of  small  social  units  and  those  of  larger  organizations 
could  be  schematically  represented  somewhat  as  follows.  Figure  4 
could  represent  the  loyalties  of  a  typical  American  factory  worker 
in  1946.  He  will  have  definite  loyalties  to  his  family;  these  may 
conflict  with  his  loyalties  to  his  union  (as,  for  example,  when  his 
family  suffers  if  he  goes  out  on  strike) ;  the  loyalties  to  his  union 
may  oppose  some  loyalty  and  identification  he  may  have  to  the 
industry  in  which  he  works;  the  precepts  he  has  subscribed  to  as 
a  Christian  he  will  find  at  times  contradicted  by  racial  discrimina- 
Union  Public  allegiance 
Discrimination  " 


Family 

Religion  (^* 

Management 

FIG.  4.    Conflict  of  values  and  identifications. 

tion  which  he  may  practice  or  condone;  if  he  thinks  of  the  con- 
sumers' welfare  or  of  the  progress  of  the  "nation  as  a  whole"  he 
may  feel  that  some  of  the  organized  pressures  which  he  has  to 
exert  along  with  other  workers  in  order  to  maintain  or  improve 
his  position  as  a  worker  are  in  conflict  with  the  over-all  welfare  of 
the  public  of  which  he  is  a  member,  and  so  on.  Just  what  con- 
flicts become  emphasized,  what  degrees  of  overlapping  in  values 
occur,  what  identifications  are  strongest  at  the  moment  will  be 
likely  to  vary  enormously  with  the  changing  demands  of  situa- 
tions. In  brief,  there  is  no  clear-cut  overlapping  of  the  values  that 
have  become  part  of  him. 

By  contrast,  in  a  situation  where  there  is  no  contradiction  either 
between  the  values  of  different  groups  or  between  the  values  of 
single  groups  and  a  larger  social  organization,  the  possibility 
exists  of  complete  overlapping  so  that  what  is  socially  important 
can  and  will  become  personally  important  and  what  is  personally 
important  can  and  will  be  socially  important.  The  schematic  pic- 
ture would  then  be  somewhat  as  in  Figure  5. 

It  is  clear  from  all  this  that  the  common  cliche  "human  nature 
can't  be  changed"  has  no  basis  in  fact.  "Human  nature"  is  incon- 
ceivable unless  it  develops  in  some  social  environment.  Any  social 


REFERENCES  383 


environment  includes  values  or  norms  which  vary  in  their  uni- 
formity and  their  duration.  What  the  individual  is  and  what  he 
feels  himself  to  be  are  largely  conditioned  by  the  particular  con- 


FIG.  5.    Overlapping  of  values  and  identifications. 

stellation  of  values  he  learns  and  that  become  a  part  of  him.  To 
begin  with,  "human  nature"  as  represented  by  any  single  indi- 
vidual or  any  group  of  individuals  is  not  something  that  follows 
any  fixed  pattern  laid  down  by  inexorable  laws  of  nature;  it  is, 
rather,  a  complex  of  potentialities  subject  to  enormous  differentia- 
tions, emerging  in  new  forms  and  evolving  to  new  adjustments  in 
a  constantly  changing  world. 

REFERENCES 

1.  ALLPORT,  G.  W.,  The  psychology  of  participation,  PsychoL  Rev.,  1945,  53, 

117-32. 

2.  AUDIENCE  RESEARCH  INSTITUTE,  INC.,  Increasing  Profits  with  Continuous  Audi- 

ence Research,  New  York:  Am.  Book,  1941;  reprinted  by  permission  of  G.  H. 
Gallup. 

3.  BUCK,  P.  S.,  Tal{  About  Russia  with  Masha  Scott,  New  York:  John  Day, 

copyright  1945. 

4.  CANTRIL,  H.,  The  prediction  of  social  events,  /.  Abnorm.  &  Soc.  Psychol.,  1938, 

33,  364-89. 

5. The  Invasion  from  Mars,  Princeton:  Univ.  Press,  1940. 

6. The  Psychology  of  Social  Movements,  New  York:  Wiley,  1941. 

7. How  the  Miners  Feel,  unpublished  report,  Princeton,  1943. 

8.  CARLYLE,  T.,  Sartor  Resartus,  Oxford:  Clarendon,  copyright  1913;  first  pub- 

lished in  Fraser's  Magazine,  1833—34. 

9.  CENSUS,  BUREAU  OF,  Industrial  Distribution  of  the  Nation's  Labor  Force:  1870 

to  1930,  October  23, 1938. 

10.  CENTERS,  R.,  Psychological  aspects  of  socio-economic  stratification:  an  enquiry 

into  the  nature  of  class,  Princeton  Univ.:  thesis  to  be  published. 

11.  COOLEY,  C.  H.,  Social  Process,  New  York:  Scribner's,  1918. 


384  EGO-INVOLVEMENTS  IN  CONCRETE  SOCIAL  SITUATIONS 

12.  DAVIS,  A.,  B.  B.  GARDNER,  and  M.  R.  GARDNER,  Deep  South,  Chicago:  Univ. 

Press,  copyright  1941. 

13.  DOBB,  M.,  Soviet  Planning  and  Labor  in  Peace  and  War,  New  York:  Inter- 

national, 1943. 

14.  FLUGEL,  J.  C.,  The  Psychology  of  Clothes,  London:  Hogarth,  copyright  1930. 

15.  FRIEDRICH,  G.,  Miss  U.S.S.R.,  New  York:  International. 

16.  GRINKER,  R.  R.,  and  J.  P.  SPIEGEL,  Men  Under  Stress,  Philadelphia:  Blakiston, 

1945. 
17. War  Neuroses,  Philadelphia:  Blakiston,  1945. 

18.  HOLT,  R.  R.,  Studies  in  the  Psychology  of  Names,  thesis  on  file  Princeton  Univ. 

Library,  1939. 

19.  HURLOCK,  E.  B.,  Motivation  in  fashion,  Arch.  PsychoL,  1929,  17,  no.  111. 

20.  JOHNSON,  H.,  The  Secret  of  Soviet  Strength,  New  York:  Workers  Library, 

copyright  1943. 

21.  LABOR,  DEPARTMENT  OF,  Incentive-Wage  Plans  and  Collective  Bargaining, 

Washington:  1942,  Bull.  717. 

22.  LEVY-BRUHL,  L.,  How  Natives  Thin\,  London:  Allen  and  Unwin,  copyright 

1926. 

23.  LEWIN,  K.,  The  Relative  Effectiveness  of  a  Lecture  Method  and  a  Method  of 

Group  Decision  for  Changing  Food  Habits,  State  Univ.  Iowa,  Child  Welfare 
Research  Station,  1942. 

24.  Mr.  Lincoln's  formula,  Fortune,  February  1944,  143-206. 

25.  LINCOLN,  J.  F.,  Intelligent  Selfishness  and  Manufacturing,  Cleveland:  Lincoln 

Electric  Co.,  1942. 

26.  LITTLEPAGE,  J.  D.,  and  D.  BESS,  In  Search  of  Soviet  Gold,  New  York:  Harcourt, 

Brace,  1937. 

27.  MACDONALD,  L.,  Labor  Problems  and  the  American  Scene,  New  York:  Harper, 

copyright  1938. 

28.  MANGUM,  W.  R.,  Class  and  Status  Determinants  within  the  Negro  Caste,  thesis 

on  file  Princeton  Univ.  Library,  1946. 

29.  MAULDIN,  B.,  Up  Front,  New  York:  Henry  Holt,  copyright  1945. 

30.  MCGREGOR,  D.,  Conditions  of  effective  leadership  in  the  industrial  organization, 

/.  Consult.  Psychol,  1944,  8,  55-63. 

31.  MOLOTOV,  V.  M.,  What  is  Stakhanovism? ,  New  York:  International,  copyright 

1936. 

32.  MYRDAL,  G.,  'An  American  Dilemma,  New  York:  Harper,  copyright  1944. 

33.  NATIONAL  INDUSTRIAL  CONFERENCE  BOARD,   INC.,    Wage  Incentive  Practices, 

Studies  in  Personnel  Policy,  No.  68,  published  by  The  Conference  Board, 
247  Park  Avenue,  New  York  17,  N.  Y.,  1945. 

34.  NATIONAL  RESEARCH  COUNCIL,  Fatigue  of  Workers — Its  Relation  to  Industrial 

Problems,  New  York:  Reinhold,  copyright  1941. 

35.  New  Yor^  Times,  March  18,  1942,  statement  by  President  on  ordering  Mac- 

Arthur  to  leave  the  Philippines. 

36. April  26,  1946,  order  issued  by  McNarney  to  all  unit  commanders  of 

American  troops  in  occupied  Germany. 

37.  NEWELL,  T.  E.,  Ncuropsychiatry  in  the  Japanese  Army,  /.  Am.  Med.  Assoc., 

1944,  126,  373  f. 

38.  Reader's  Digest,  June  1937,  108,  anonymous. 


REFERENCES  385 

39.  ROETHLISBERGER,  F.  J.,  and  W.  J.  DICKSON,  Management  and  the  Worker, 

Cambridge:  Harvard  Univ.  Press,  copyright  1943. 

40.  RUBINSTEIN,  S.,  Soviet  psychology  in  wartime,  Phil,  and  Phenom.  Res.,  1944,  5, 

181-98. 

41.  SHERIF,  M.,  The  Psychology  of  Social  Norms,  New  York:  Harper,  1936. 

42.  STALIN,  J.,  The  Statyanov  Movement  in  the  Soviet  Union,  New  York:  Work- 

ers Library,  copyright  1935. 

43.  VEBLEN,  T.,  The  Theory  of  the  Leisure  Class,  New  York:  Modern  Library, 

copyright  1934;  quotations  by  permission  of  Viking  Press. 

44.  WARD,  H.  R,  The  Soviet  Spirit,  New  York:  International,  copyright  1944. 

45.  WATSON,  G.,  Work  satisfaction,  in  Industrial  Conflict:  A  Psychological  Inter- 

pretation (G.  W.  Hartmann  and  T.  Newcomb,  eds.),  New  York:  Cordon, 
1939,  ch.  6. 

46.  WATSON,  W.  T.,  Division  of  Labor:  A  Study  in  the  Sociology  and  the  Social 

Psychology  of  Wor1{  Satisfaction,  Ph.D.  dissertation,  Univ.  Chicago,  1930. 

47.  WILSON,  N.  F.,  Charlie  Wilson,  businessman,  Adventures  in  Business,  1944,  II. 


CHAPTER  12 
BREAKDOWNS  OF  THE  EGO 

The  ego  cannot  be  regarded  as  a  fixed,  rigid,  or  permanent  en- 
tity. For  no  matter  how  well  formed  or  "integrated"  the  ego  may 
seem  to  be,  it  can  be  and  sometimes  is  considerably  altered  by 
stresses  and  strains  and  upheavals  of  one  sort  or  another.  These 
pressures  may  be  due  either  to  the  impact  of  concrete  situations 
which  have  various  degrees  of  compellingness,  or  they  may  be 
rooted  in  the  physical  organism  and  due  to  motivational  (instinc- 
tual) tensions  or  to  organic  disturbances.  Even  though  an  indi- 
vidual builds  up  a  relatively  well-formed  ego  in  the  course  of  life, 
even  though  he  may  have  a  number  of  firmly  established  ego- 
attitudes  which  determine  or  affect  his  judgment  and  behavior, 
even  though  his  particular  constellation  of  ego-involved  values 
has  a  fairly  stable  continuity  of  personal  identity,  and  even 
though  the  individual  experiences  the  continuous  flow  of  his  own 
identity  from  day  to  day  and  from  year  to  year,  and  others  around 
him  have  consistent  expectations  as  to  what  he  will  or  will  not  do 
under  various  conditions — in  spite  of  all  this — sufficient  pressure 
of  one  kind  or  another  can  break  down  the  ego  in  varying  degrees. 

There  will,  of  course,  be  wide  individual  differences  in  the  par- 
ticular way  any  given  person  is  affected  by  the  same  pressure  or 
pattern  of  forces.  We  have  repeatedly  pointed  out  that  ego-con- 
stellations vary  according  to  social  and  personal  values  and  that 
they  vary  not  only  in  their  range  of  inclusiveness  but  also  in  their 
intensity.  Undeniable  individual  differences  in  temperament,  abil- 
ity, and  organic  functions  all  affect  ego  stability.  Nevertheless, 
the  variety  of  individual  differences,  whatever  their  cause,  in  no 
way  alters  the  fact  that  the  ego  can  be  radically  changed;  can 
break  down;  can  expand,  shrink,  dissociate,  or  regress  if  a  situa- 
tion is  so  compelling  as  to  be  crushing,  if  the  deprivation  of  an 
instinctual  need  is  so  severe  as  to  be  "unbearable,"  or  if  an  organic 

386 


DISSOCIATION  OF  THE  EGO  FROM  THE  BODY  387 

disturbance  is  so  great  as  to  make  "normal*'  behavior  impossible. 
The  whole  problem  of  the  breakdown  of  the  ego,  and  we  repeat 
again,  an  ego  that  is  genetically  formed,  deserves  much  more  de- 
tailed and  elaborate  consideration  than  space  allows  here.  But  in 
this  chapter  we  can  at  least  indicate  with  examples,  some  of  the 
circumstances  under  which  the  ego  is  altered  in  one  way  or  an- 
other, beginning  with  relatively  normal  instances  and  ending  with 
pathological  cases. 

DISSOCIATION  OF  THE  EGO  FROM  THE  BODY 

The  child  has  to  learn  and  does  learn  that  his  body  is  part  of 
himself.  And  he  learns  the  norms  that  constitute  modesty,  physi- 
cal attractiveness,  and  morality.  A  constellation  of  values,  both 
social  and  personal,  associated  with  his  body — its  appearance,  well- 
being,  sanctity — become  an  integral  part  of  the  genetically  formed 
ego  (ch.  7).  We  feel  inferior  if  our  physical  attractiveness, 
strength,  or  poise  is  not  more  or  less  up  to  standard;  we  feel  em- 
barrassed if  we  overhear  someone  say  that  we  smell  badly  or  don't 
keep  our  fingernails  clean. 

In  her  revealing  autobiography  [10],  Sheila  Cousins,  a  London 
prostitute,  pointed  clearly  to  the  distinction  between  her  body  as 
an  integral  part  of  her  person  and  her  body  as  something  men 
used. 

I  have  moments  when  I  realize  that  I  am  a  person  to  no  one,  that 
to  the  male  I  am  just  a  body,  to  the  policeman  a  chance  of  promotion, 
to  the  rest  simply  a  problem.  [2] 

However  lofty  their  protestations,  men  were,  in  the  end,  only  after 
my  body.  [10,  145] 

"I  wasn't  a  good  prostitute,"  she  writes.  "I  hadn't  sufficient  de- 
tachment from  what  I  was  doing  to  be  able  to  pretend  feeling  for 
my  clients  ...  the  act  of  sex  I  could  go  through  because  /  hardly 
seemed  to  be  taking  fart  In  it.  It  was  merely  something  happen- 
ing to  me,  while  my  mind  drifted  inconsequentially  away.  In- 
deed, //  was  scarcely  happening  even  to  me:  it  was  happening  to 
something  lying  on  a  bed  that  had  a  vague  connection  with  me, 
while  I  was  calculating  whether  I  could  afford  a  new  coat  or 


388  BREAKDOWNS  OF  THE  EGO 

impatiently  counting  sheep  jumping  over  a  gate!'  [10,  150  f., 
italics  ours] 

Personal  interviews  with  ten  prostitutes  in  New  York  City 
confirmed  Cousins'  observations  that,  when  participating  in  the  sex 
act  as  a  prostitute,  a  woman,  as  a  person f  does  not  usually  seem  to 
be  involved.1  Of  the  ten  young  women  interviewed,  only  two  said 
that  they  put  "themselves"  into  the  sex  act  when  being  paid  for  it. 
Both  of  them  described  themselves  as  "over-sexed."  For  the  other 
eight,  any  "enjoyment"  was  only  a  pretense  put  on  for  business 
reasons.  Here  are  some  typical  statements  from  the  conversations 
with  prostitutes: 

The  less  personal  you  become  the  easier  it  is  on  you. 

I  think  I  can  honestly  say  that  any  emotions  I  might  display  to  a 
man  are  just  pretense.  /  never  become  aroused.  Afterwards  I  am 
tired  and  a  great  deal  disgusted  with  the  horrible  habits  men  acquire. 
But  I  guess  I  am  no  better  than  they  are  or  I  wouldn't  be  with  them. 

I  almost  always  pretend  that  I  am  having  some  sort  of  response. 
After  all  these  men  are  paying  money  to  be  kidded.  The  least  I  can 
do  is  see  to  it  that  they  are  not  cheated.  How  good  an  act  do  I  put  on  ? 
I  suppose  all  the  things  we  do  in  this  are  acting  to  some  extent.  I  kid 
and  laugh  with  them  before  I  get  down  to  real  business,  this  makes 
them  think  I'm  a  pretty  good  sport,  but  believe  me  sometimes  I  could 
choke  them.  I  don't  care  what  anyone  says,  it's  not  as  easy  as  some 
people  think. 

Do  I  put  myself  into  it?  No,  I  don't.  Any  man  who  has  to  go  out 
and  pay  for  it,  is  not  worth  the  effort  it  would  take  to  try  and  respond. 

//  doesn't  mean  anything  at  all  to  me.  How  could  it  mean  anything 
when  you  make  your  living  at  it?  This  is  a  business  just  like  any 
other.  You  can't  be  sentimental  in  this  racket.  The  men  want  you 
to  go  down  and  that  is  all.  So  you  get  as  much  as  you  can  out  of 
them  and  put  as  little  as  you  can  into  it. 

But  when  these  prostitutes  were  with  their  husbands  (and  quite 
a  number  of  them  were  married)  or  went  out  with  their  sweet- 
hearts or  boy  friends,  the  sex  act  took  on  a  very  different  charac- 
ter; the  women  reported  that  they  took  part  in  it  as  individuals, 
that  it  was  happening  to  them,  not  just  to  their  bodies. 

1  These  interviews  were  all  made  privately  and  confidentially  by  a  young  woman 
personally  acquainted  with  the  prostitutes.  Her  assistance  was  obtained  through 
the  kind  interest  of  a  New  York  physician. 


DISSOCIATION  OF  THE  EGO  FROM  THE  BODY  389 

Sheila  Cousins  reports  her  feelings  after  she  first  met  Richard 
whom  she  later  married. 

I  liked  Richard.  I  felt  at  once  he  wasn't  xmt  to  buy  me.  That  first 
day  he  got  me  a  pair  of  gunmetal  stockings  and  a  pair  of  gloves  after 
lunch,  but  I  knew  that  there  would  be  no  account  to  settle  in  a 
fumbling  taxi  going  home.  There  was  a  friendly  shyness  about  every- 
thing he  said  or  did.  ...  I  discovered  that  he  was  not  only  in  love 
with  me,  he  respected  me.  I  think  he  was  the  first  man  who  ever  had. 
[10, 136  f.] 

In  a  life  where  sex  is  merely  a  commodity,  casually  traded  over  the 
counterpane  daily,  the  faintest  spark  of  personal  affection  grows  to  be 
worth  a  fortune.  [10,  182] 

The  same  thing  appears  in  the  conversations  with  New  York 
prostitutes.  When  they  go  out  with  their  lovers  or  boy  friends, 
they  indulge  in  characteristic  feminine  resistances.  Their  egos  are 
involved,  and  they  want  to  defend  them  with  the  feminine  wiles 
appropriate  to  the  norms  of  their  society.  And  if  they  do  have 
intercourse,  their  reaction  is  more  complete,  they  feel  involved. 

When  I  meet  a  man  whom  I  would  really  like  as  a  lover,  I  flirt 
with  him.  If  you  choose  a  man,  that  does  not  mean  he  is  necessarily 
attracted  to  you.  So  it  is  only  natural  that  you  have  to  assert  yourself 
in  order  to  interest  him  in  you.  When  I  have  finished  with  a  man 
who  pays  me,  I  feel  tired.  I  feel  like  leaving  him  as  soon  as  possible. 
But  when  I  am  with  my  boy-friend  it's  so  different.  He  comes  to  my 
house.  I  always  meet  the  other  men  outside.  When  my  boy-friend 
comes  we  make  a  real  time  of  it.  We  eat  and  drink  and  fool  around. 
We  talk  and  have  fun.  I  flirt  with  him  for  I  want  him  to  keep 
interested  in  me. 

I  have  many  affairs  with  men  who  do  not  pay  me.  //  a  man  is  nice 
and  I  am  attracted  to  him  I  see  no  reason  why  I  should  not  indulge 
myself.  I  don't  believe  I  flirt  with  them  openly  but  I  do  try  to  attract 
them.  Doesn't  every  woman  try  to  attract  men  to  her  with  what  she 
considers  her  feminine  guiles?  As  for  any  such  affair  being  auto- 
matic, that  is  for  animals,  not  people. 

The  same  lack  of  ego-involvement — the  separation  of  the  physi- 
cal needs  of  the  body  from  the  self— can  be  seen  from  the  accounts 
of  men  describing  sex  relations  they  have  had  with  women  who 
were  not  their  sweethearts  or  wives.  Here  is  an  account  of  the  sex 


390  BREAKDOWNS  OF  THE  EGO 

life  of  officers  and  men  overseas  with  the  U.  S.  Army  during 
World  War  II  written  for  us  by  an  American  officer  who  spent 
nearly  four  years  abroad. 

During  my  time  overseas  I  talked  intimately  with  many  officers  and 
men  of  the  American  Army.  Most  of  them  were  in  their  twenties. 
Quite  a  number  were  married,  many  not  long  before  they  had  been 
ordered  abroad.  Although  some  of  these  marriages  were  either  on  the 
rocks  or  obviously  about  to  break  up,  the  majority  of  the  boys  seemed 
very  much  in  love  with  their  wives.  They  would  talk  to  me  for  hours 
about  their  wives,  their  children  if  they  had  any,  and  their  plans  for 
the  future  of  their  family. 

Frequently,  however,  the  morning  after  one  of  these  talks,  they 
would  tell  me  in  jocose  fashion  about  some  gal  they  had  slept  with 
the  night  before.  There  was  no  embarrassment  whatever  about  such 
confessions  and  probably  a  few  hours  later  they  would  be  telling  me 
again  how  much  they  loved  their  wives. 

The  explanation,  I  am  sure,  lies  in  the  complete  dissociation  in  the 
minds  of  these  particular  soldiers  between  their  wives  and  families,  on 
the  one  handt  and  the  necessity  of  relieving  their  glandular  pressures 
on  the  other.  In  their  eyes,  there  is  no  connection  between  these  two 
things.  While  the  man  might  sleep  temporarily — and  perforce  by 
necessity — with  sluts  and  prostitutes,  there  is  never  any  doubt  in  his 
mind  where  his  love  and  major  values  lie:  with  his  wife  and  his  home. 
In  following  the  necessitous  path  of  extramarital  relationships,  he  does 
not  feel  that  he  is  doing  his  wife  any  harm  (short  of  contracting  a 
disease),  and  feels  quite  clear  that  his  behavior  has  nothing  to  do  with 
what  will  be  his  life  when  he  returns  home  again.  This  is  not  true, 
of  course,  for  all  soldiers,  but  did  seem  to  be  the  state  of  affairs  for 
most  of  those  who  indulged  in  extramarital  relations. 

All  of  this  points  again  to  the  ancient  and  well-recognized 
conclusion  that  the  physical  sex  act  can  be  regarded  as  an  act  of 
"love"  in  proportion  to  the  extent  the  partners  merge  their  egos, 
identify  themselves  with  each  other.  Van  de  Velde  has  expressed 
it  in  his  famous  book  Ideal  Marriage: 

What  husband  and  wife  who  love  one  another  seek  to  achieve  in 
their  most  intimate  bodily  communion,  and,  whether  consciously  or 
unconsciously,  recognize  as  the  purpose  of  such  communion  is:  a 
means  of  expression  that  makes  them  One.  [42,  321] 


THE  EGO  MAY  TEMPORARILY  DISSOLVE  391 

THE  EGO  MAY  TEMPORARILY  DISSOLVE 

Under  certain  conditions  an  individual  may  merge  his  own  ego 
with  another  person's,  feel  that  he — as  a  single  identifiable  person 
— has  ceased  to  exist  and  that  he  belongs  to  and  is  a  part  of  some- 
one else.  In  the  next  chapter  we  cite  illustrations  from  literature 
of  this  mergence  of  one  ego  with  another,  of  lovers  who,  together, 
feel  as  one  but  who,  alone  or  separated,  feel  personally  torn  apart. 
This  same  melting  away  of  the  ego  is  also  experienced  by  certain 
creative  artists  who  "lose  themselves"  in  their  work.  The  dancer, 
Martha  Graham,  has  said,  for  example,  that  an  artist  must  first 
"destroy  himself  if  he  is  to  be  creative.  [5,  142]  In  elaborating 
this  statement  of  Martha  Graham's,  a  commentator  writes: 

It  was  not  merely  a  matter  of  sloughing  off  this  manner  and  that 
technique.    A  whole  self  had  to  be  danced  out  and  away.  [5,  142] 

The  ego  may  also  temporarily  "break  down"  under  the  influence 
of  alcohol  or  various  drugs.  There  is  an  apt  definition  of  the 
"superego"  as  "that  portion  of  the  personality  which  is  soluble  in 
alcohol."  Since  we  will  indicate  later  (pp.  492  /.)  that  there  seems 
to  us  no  reason  to  use  the  term  superego  as  something  distinct 
from  the  ego,  we  can  just  as  well  apply  this  definition  to  the  ego 
itself.  Everyone  knows  that  when  a  man  gets  drunk  he  is  likely 
to  say  or  do  things  which  surprise  those  who  know  him  well. 
After  consuming  sufficient  alcohol,  a  "gentleman"  or  "lady"  may 
clearly  lose  some  of  his  normal  behavioral  characteristics  because 
some  of  the  restraining  influences  of  his  ego — personal  or  social 
values  which  are  part  of  himself— have  collapsed.  In  the  inter- 
views with  prostitutes,  we  have  seen  how  these  young  women 
often  dissociated  their  bodies  from  their  persons  in  their  work. 
The  prostitutes  were  also  asked  if  alcohol  had  any  effect  on  the 
ease  with  which  they  could  give  their  bodies  to  a  customer. 
Among  those  who  drank,  the  consensus  of  opinion  was  that  a  few 
drinks  before  intercourse  for  business  did  facilitate  separation  of 
the  "self"  from  the  body.  Here  are  some  of  their  statements. 

Drinking  to  me  is  a  boon.    I  naturally  am  a  reticent  person,  and 
alcohol  brings  me  out  of  myself.    I  become  quite  bold. 


J92  BREAKDOWNS  OF  THE  EGO 

Another  reports: 

Alcohol  makes  me  feel  like  being  more  entertaining.  Some  of  the 
girls  get  all  ginned  up,  so  that  they  can  take  anything  that  comes 
along. 

Another  girl  said: 

This  job  of  mine  is  not  as  easy  as  some  people  think,  so  a  little  nip 
now  and  then  does  the  trick. 

Those  who  have  studied  inveterate  drinkers  known  as  alcoholics 
seem  generally  agreed  that  the  underlying  reasons  for  the  addic- 
tion is  its  acceptance  by  the  individual  as  a  method  of  escaping  or 
overcoming  some  unpleasant  situation  or  emotional  conflict,  or  its 
use  to  make  it  possible  for  an  individual  to  play  some  role  denied 
him  in  normal  life.  [33,  ch.  12]  A  penetrating  analysis  of  alco- 
holics has  been  written  by  Bales  who  states  that 

The  alcoholic  is  a  man  divided  against  himself.  No  matter  how 
genuinely  he  may  agree  with  those  who  condemn  him,  there  is  another 
part  of  him  which  fights  back  .  .  .  the  drinker  becomes  more  and 
more  isolated,  "desperately  alone,"  "misunderstood,"  more  and  more 
bound  up  with  his  own  circular  reasoning  and  self-destructive  tend- 
ency. .  .  .  The  alcoholic  "loses  his  perspective,"  as  some  of  them  put 
it,  and  finds  himself  "bucking  the  whole  world."  [4,  16] 

It  might  be  mentioned  in  passing  and  in  connection  with  the 
function  of  groups  as  described  in  chapter  10  and  the  overlapping 
of  identifications  discussed  in  chapter  11  that  the  most  successful 
cures  for  alcoholics  seem  to  be  those  which  enable  the  individual 
to  reintegrate  his  broken  ego  "with  a  social  group  of  which  he 
feels  truly  and  basically  a  part,  a  group  which  understands  him 
thoroughly  and  sympathetically."  [4,  17]  The  current  success  of 
the  organization  known  as  Alcoholics  Anonymous  seems  due,  for 
example,  almost  entirely  to  the  fact  that  the  members  of  the 
organization  are  all  ex-alcoholics,  that  "the  new  candidate  in  such 
a  group  intuitively  recognizes  that  he  is  among  friends"  [18]  and 
that  in  such  a  group  "the  alcoholic  obtains  recognition  and  re- 
sponse through  the  admission  of  thoughts  and  activities  which, 
before,  he  had  been  desperately  trying  to  hide,  even  from  him- 


BREAKDOWNS  OF  TOE  EGO  UNDER  EXTREME  DEPRIVATION         393 

sqlf."  [18]   In  summarizing  the  success  of  Alcoholics  Anonymous, 
Bales  writes: 

As  a  member  of  a  solidary  group  in  which  the  desired  ideas  are 
held  as  group  convictions,  they  come  home  to  the  individual  with  a 
personalized,  tailored-to-fit  quality,  and  with  an  emotional  intensity 
and  repetition  impossible  to  duplicate  in  any  other  way.  In  the  matrix 
of  a  concrete  group  with  which  the  individual  is  closely  identified,  and 
in  which  he  has  a  particular  role,  the  ideas  and  desired  behavior  pat- 
terns can  be  thoroughly  integrated  with  his  social  goals,  tied  up  imme- 
diately and  directly  with  those  emotions  and  needs  which  can  only  be 
activated  and  satisfied  in  a  social  context.  [4,  21] 

In  describing  the  use  of  the  drug  pentothal  as  an  aid  in  relieving 
neuroses  created  by  war  experiences,  Grinker  and  Spiegel  speak 
of  "the  sedative  effect  of  the  drug  on  the  ego"  [22,  436].  and  state 
that  it  "serves  to  desensitize  the  ego."  [383]  Without  necessarily 
accepting  the  psychoanalytic  implications  of  these  authors,  we  find 
their  description  of  the  effect  of  the  drug  highly  pertinent  to  our 
discussion  here.  For  they  point  out  how  pentothal,  combined  with 
intelligent  psychotherapy,  can  bring  together  different  portions  of 
the  self  into  a  more  normally  integrated  ego.  Under  the  influence 
of  pentothal  "the  relationship  between  regressive  dependent  needs 
and  self-respecting  ego  forces  and  even  overcompensations  often 
becomes  quite  clear  as  the  different  portions  of  the  personality  are 
expressed  in  associations."  [391] 

Under  the  influence  of  the  drug  and  during  the  process  of  abreac- 
tion,  although  not  fully  conscious,  the  ego,  devoid  of  the  stress  of 
anxiety,  synthesizes  some  and  often  much  of  the  important  isolated 
and  pathogenic  material  into  its  main  body.  It  is  as  if  the  emotions  or 
the  memories  had  been  separated  from  the  active  ego  forces  as  in  a 
hysterical  dissociation,  because  they  had  been  too  threatening  to  the 
ego's  stability  or  productive  of  terrifying  and  unendurable  anxiety. 
[22,  392  f.] 

BREAKDOWNS  OF  THE  EGO  UNDER  EXTREME  DEPRIVATION 

If  an  individual  is  long  denied  the  satisfaction  of  an  instinctual 
need,  then  there  is  a  considerably  greater  chance  that  he  will 
transgress  the  established  norms  which  under  more  usual  condi- 


394  BREAKDOWNS  OF  THE  EGO 

tions  are  part  of  his  ideas  of  good  conduct  and  with  which  he 
would  normally  identify  himself.  Enforced  sexual  continence 
may  lead  to  such  results  as  obscene  acts  and  homosexuality;  pro- 
longed hunger  may  lead  to  theft  or  to  some  other  form  of  anti- 
social or  at  least  undignified  behavior. 

Bill  Mauldin,  in  describing  the  "abject  poverty  and  hunger  of 
the  Italian  refugees"  encountered  by  the  United  States  Armies  in 
Italy,  has  pointed  out  the  very  unladylike  behavior  of  old  Italian 
women  under  such  conditions. 

It  would  take  a  pretty  tough  guy  not  to  feel  his  heart  go  out  to  the 
shivering,  little  six-year-old  squeaker  who  stands  barefoot  in  the  mud, 
holding  a  big  tin  bucket  so  the  dogface  can  empty  his  mess  kit  into  it. 
Many  soldiers,  veterans  of  the  Italy  campaign  and  thousands  of  similar 
buckets,  still  go  back  and  sweat  out  the  mess  line  for  an  extra  chop 
and  hunk  of  bread  for  those  little  kids.  But  there  is  a  big  difference 
between  the  ragged,  miserable  infantryman  who  waits  with  his  mess 
kit,  and  the  ragged,  miserable  civilian  who  waits  with  his  bucket. 
The  doggie  knows  where  his  next  meal  is  coming  from.  That  makes 
him  a  very  rich  man  in  this  land  where  hunger  is  so  fierce  it  maizes 
animals  out  of  respectable  old  ladies  who  should  be  wearing  cameos 
and  having  tea  parties  instead  of  fighting  one  another  savagely  for  a 
soldier's  scraps.  [31,  66  ff.f  italics  ours] 

Another  American  soldier  told  how  norms  of  decency  broke 
down  in  Italy  and  Sicily  to  such  an  extent  that  formerly  respectable 
women  sent  children  out  on  the  streets  to  solicit  Allied  soldiers 
for  them,  to  circumvent  the  stringent  penalties  imposed  on  public 
prostitution.  These  observations  are  particularly  significant  since 
the  soldier  who  reported  them  had  lived  in  Italy  for  some  time 
before  the  war,  spoke  fluent  Italian,  had  seen  the  country  under 
normal  conditions  and  knew  that  the  behavior  he  observed  was 
nothing  "typical"  of  Italy  but  due  to  the  extraordinary  conditions 
created  by  war. 

While  in  Palermo,  I  saw  little  children  soliciting  trade  for  their 
sisters,  cousins,  aunts,  or  mothers.  They  would  stop  soldiers  on  the 
street  with  such  remarks  as  "Hey,  Joe,  wanna  gal,  verra  clean,  only 
one  dollar,"  or  they  would  use  some  American  slang  to  mention  per- 
versions that  made  the  trade  sound  more  interesting.  In  one  instance, 
I  observed  a  mother  with  her  daughter  carrying  on  "trade"  in  a  single 


BREAKDOWNS  OF  THE  EGO  UNDER  EXTREME  DEPRIVATION         395 

dark  room  on  a  squalid  street  on  the  outskirts  of  the  city  with  another 
G.I.  outside,  waiting.  From  all  accounts  I  could  gather,  these  women 
had  originally  come  from  the  servant  class  who  now  found  a  more 
profitable  way  to  spend  their  time.  On  other  occasions,  I  encountered 
women  of  higher  class  means  inviting  officers  to  their  homes,  hoping 
to  acquire  some  political  favor  or  food.  Their  "trade"  was  more 
formal  and  practiced  with  little  publicity.  War  widows  or  those  who 
had  their  husbands  in  Allied  prison  camps  were  especially  among  the 
latter  type.  On  still  other  occasions,  one  found  young  girls  who  were 
out  to  have  a  "good  time"  and  the  disruption  of  family  life  spread  a 
sense  of  "devil  may  care"  attitude.  Most  of  these  women  had  been 
bombed  out  of  their  homes  and  had  lost  their  cherished  possessions. 
Many  had  had  relatives  killed.  They  had  been  forced  out  of  their 
normal  home  environments  and  the  fear  of  ostracism  from  the  social 
pattern  indigenous  to  that  environment  no  longer  existed.  It  was  no 
wonder  they  had  become  cynical,  hard-bitten  and  had  chosen  this  pro- 
fession out  of  economic  necessity.  I  had  spent  considerable  time  in 
Italy  before  the  war.  What  I  saw  during  the  war  was  a  real  shock  to 
me  because  it  was  so  very  different  from  what  I  had  previously 
known.2 

A  German  physician,  once  an  inmate  of  the  Belsen  concentration 
camp,  has  reported  how  the  ordinary  self-respecting  and  law- 
abiding  citizens  of  normal  life  "resorted  to  cannibalism  to  avoid 
starvation."  The  account  of  his  testimony  states: 

He  told  how  human  beings  fought  like  animals  for  food,  how  they 
carried  human  flesh  around  in  their  pockets,  and  how  the  SS  (Nazi 
Elite  Guard)  ordered  those  who  practiced  cannibalism  hanged  or 
beaten  to  death.  [35] 

Further  evidence  that  the  ego  is  not  a  mystical  entity,  that  it  is 
related  to  the  organism  and  affected  by  physical  deprivation  is 
seen  in  changes  of  personality  such  as  feelings  of  unusual  exhilira- 
tion  and  well-being,  loss  of  judgment,  emotional  outbursts,  the 
impairment  of  critical  ability  and  self-criticism,  if  an  individual 
breathes  air  which  lacks  the  amount  of  oxygen  normally  required 
(Anoxia).  [40,  585  f.]  Recent  studies  have  also  shown  that  even 
a  slight  deficiency  of  vitamin  B  has  marked  influences  on  a  per- 

2  These  observations  were  given  us  by  R.  V.  Botsford,  Princeton  '48,  who  was 
with  the  American  Army  in  Italy. 


396  BREAKDOWNS  OF  THE  EGO 

son's  behavior  and  the  conduct  he  would  normally  feel  befitting 
to  him.   Summarizing  a  number  of  studies,  Shock  reports: 

One  of  the  symptoms  of  even  slight  vitamin  B  deficiency  in  human 
subjects  is  their  increased  irritability,  moodiness,  lack  of  cooperation 
and  "meanness."  With  more  severe  B-vitamin  deficiency  such  mental 
states  as  apathy,  depression,  and  emotional  instability  have  often  been 
observed.  A  most  striking  study  is  that  of  Williams,  ct  al.  (1942) 
who  restricted  the  B-vitamin  (thiamin)  intake  of  eleven  women  to 
.45  mg.  daily,  which  is  an  amount  little,  if  any,  below  that  obtained 
in  many  American  diets.  Within  six  or  eight  weeks  the  subjects 
began  to  show  such  symptoms  of  emotional  instability  as  irritability, 
moodiness,  quarrelsomeness,  lack  of  cooperation,  and  vague  fears  pro- 
gressing to  agitation  and  mental  depression.  When  thiamin  was 
restored  to  their  diets,  or  even  when  riboflavin  was  reduced  in  the 
presence  of  adequate  thiamin,  no  such  symptoms  were  observed.8 
[40,59S/.] 

THE  EGO  MAY  BREAK  DOWN  IF  ESTABLISHED  NORMS  COLLAPSE  OR 
ARE  NO  LONGER  IN  FORCE 

If  an  individual  has  identified  himself,  made  part  of  himself, 
certain  social  values  which  are  suddenly  and  completely  upset  or 
destroyed  by  some  major  cataclysm,  then  that  portion  of  the  ego 
composed  of  those  values  may  itself  be  destroyed,  and  the  indi- 
vidual may  exhibit  behavior  quite  out  of  keeping  with  what  he  or 
his  friends  would  regard  as  normal  or  right  for  him.  Two  illus- 
trations, both  dealing  with  the  collapse  of  norms  in  Germany  as 
a  result  of  Allied  advances,  will  suffice  here  to  show  how  behavior 
can  be  radically  altered  when  normal  ego-involving  standards  are 
suddenly  withdrawn. 

A  vivid  description  of  the  looting  of  a  German  castle  was  written 
for  us  by  Lewis  F.  Gittler.  The  important  part  of  the  story  for  our 
present  interests  is  put  in  italics.  It  shows  how  respectable  middle- 
class  Germans  in  one  moment  condemned  the  looting  of  the  castle 
by  others,  even  by  other  Germans  who  happened  to  be  peasants, 
but  in  the  next  moment  themselves  indulged  in  looting  when  they 

8  The  study  referred  to  by  Shock  is  [45], 


EGO  MAY  BREAK  DOWN  IF  ESTABLISHED  NORMS  COLLAPSE         397 

learned  that  there  was  something  they  wanted  badly  and  could 
now  steal  without  fear  of  legal  action  or  particular  social  dis- 
approval.4 

Near  the  Weser  River  east  of  Paderborn  was  a  large  modernized 
castle  dominating  a  town  of  about  350  inhabitants.  A  cavalry  recon- 
naissance squadron  on  its  way  to  the  river  passed  by  the  castle  about 
8  P.M.  one  evening.  There  had  been  no  American  troops  in  the  town 
as  yet  and  there  was  no  resistance  on  the  part  of  the  Germans.  It  was 
an  out  of  the  way  town,  reachable  only  by  a  dirt  road  thru  a  forest. 
As  the  column  of  jeeps,  half-tracks  and  armored  cars  passed  the  castle, 
they  decided  to  spend  the  night  there.  The  major  sent  in  an  officer 
to  negotiate  with  the  Baroness.  He  asked  for  two  floors  for  his  troops. 
She  could  remain  in  her  own  quarters  and  the  upper  two  floors.  She 
began  protesting,  complaining  it  would  not  be  feasible  and  generally 
became  such  a  nuisance  about  the  thing  that  the  Major  ordered  the 
castle  evacuated  by  its  occupants  in  two  hours.  The  old  Baroness  left 
and  the  toughened  reconnaissance  troops  moved  in.  The  castle  was 
jampacked  with  goods  and  clothes  and  furniture  and  foods  of  all 
kinds.  The  troops  disturbed  nothing  outside  of  the  wine  cellar. 
There  they  found  1,000  bottles  of  French  champagne  and  cognac. 
Until  4  A.M.  they  drank  and  finally  left  at  dawn  with  every  bottle  of 
liquor,  camera,  souvenirs,  etc.  At  dawn,  the  townspeople  woke  up  to 
find  the  Americans  gone.  Many  came  back  from  the  fields  where 
they  had  hidden  in  terror.  There  was  no  authority  left  in  the  town. 
The  old  Baroness  (known  locally  as  the  Alte  Hexe,  "old  witch,")  was 
in  another  chateau  about  15  miles  away  where  Army  trucks  had  taken 
her  and  her  servants.  The  Baroness  had  been  the  power  and  the 
mainstay  in  that  village.  Her  grounds  and  acreage  had  about  200 
Poles  and  Russian  slave  laborers. 

I  had  come  in  with  the  reconnaissance  squadron  and  after  they  left 
I  stayed  behind.  At  about  6  A.M.,  the  Russians  came  into  the  courtyard 
and  seeing  no  one  around  went  into  the  castle  and  came  out  loaded 
down  with  clothing.  The  Poles  were  next.  They  went  in  and  came 
out  with  hundreds  of  jars  of  conserved  foods.  The  German  peasants 
went  in  next  and  carted  out  lamps  and  bedding.  In  the  house  where 
I  was  staying  were  several  refugees  from  Krefeld  and  Duesseldorf. 
They  were  of  the  substantial  middle  class  and  had  used  this  as  their 
summer  house.  One  was  an  art  dealer,  another  a  soap  manufacturer, 

4  This  incident  was  first  reported  to  us  by  Donald  V.  McGranahan,  former 
major,  AUS,  who  suggested  we  get  in  touch  with  Mr.  Gittlcr. 


398  BREAKDOWNS  OF  THE  EGO 

another  a  publisher.  We  watched  the  looting  going  on  and  as  it 
became  more  and  more  intense,  these  Germans  were  outraged  by  the 
filthy  Poles  and  the  thieving  Russians  and  the  stupid  peasants.  They 
talked  about  dignity  and  honor  and  how  Americans  should  prevent 
this  sort  of  thing  and  if  the  Alte  Hexe  comes  back  there  would  be 
hell  to  pay.  Just  then,  the  sister  of  one  of  the  men  in  the  house  came 
in  breathlessly  saying,  "Someone  just  opened  the  big  room  with  the 
silverware!'  It  had  been  known  in  the  town  that  the  hexe  had  thou- 
sands of  Solingen  flat  ware  in  a  special  room,  and  all  kinds  of  kitchen 
utensils,  boxes  and  boxes  of  them.  The  soap  manufacturer  and  the 
art  dealer  nodded  and  a  moment  later  muttered  something  about  going 
downstairs.  I  saw  them  go  into  the  castle  with  a  dozen  other  re- 
spectable-looping Germans.  A  minute  later  they  came  out  with  their 
arms  full  of  canopeners,  kjiives,  ]or\$,  scissors  and  pots.  The  cycle  of 
looting  was  completed  and  the  castle  was  quiet  again  with  only  a  few 
Poles  scavenging  around. 

Members  of  the  German  army  during  World  War  II  were 
generally  regarded  as  highly  disciplined  troops,  with  great  love 
and  respect  for  the  Fatherland.  However,  documents  found  on 
German  troops,  captured  during  the  last  months  of  1944  after  the 
Germans  had  been  pushed  back  on  their  own  territory  and  when 
it  was  becoming  clear  that  defeat  was  inevitable,  revealed  warnings 
from  German  commanders  against  looting  by  German  soldiers  on 
German  territory.  The  documents  indicate  in  no  uncertain  terms 
that  the  considerable  looting  going  on  during  that  period  was 
entirely  inconsistent  with  the  duty  and  honor  of  a  German  soldier.8 

In  the  last  chapter  we  noted  that  some  individuals  identify  them- 
selves so  completely  with  certain  established  values  that  they  prefer 
to  die  the  death  of  a  martyr  or  a  hero  rather  than  live  in  a  world 
in  which  these  values  are  taken  away.  Such  people  refuse  to 
compromise,  and  by  destroying  themselves  they  preserve  what  they 
regard  as  their  integrity.  For  them  there  is  no  breakdown,  no 
dissociation,  no  alteration  of  the  ego.  The  implication  is  that  those 
who  do  not  follow  the  logical  course  of  the  martyr  must  make 
readjustments  if  they  are  to  live  in  peace  with  themselves.  As 
Dublin  and  Bunzel  remark  in  their  study  of  suicide  [11,  281]  "self- 

6  The  writers  wish  to  thank  Donald  V.  McGranahan  for  sending  these  captured 
documents. 


EGO  MAY  BECOME  RE-FORMED  UNDER  SITUATIONS  OF  STRESS     399 

destruction  seems  the  only  solution  of  intolerable  difficulties,"  and 
they  cite  notes  left  behind  by  suicides  which  show  how  impossible 
it  has  been  for  them  either  to  alter  themselves  or  escape  from 
themselves. 

Bettelheim  has  described  the  case  of  a  prominent  German 
politician  who  had  been  kept  in  a  concentration  camp  by  the  Nazis 
and  who  saw  that  adaptation  to  the  camp  was  only  possible  if  a 
basic  change  occurred  in  his  personality  and  value  relationships. 
The  man  killed  himself  rather  than  live  on  as  essentially  another 
person. 

He  declared  that  according  to  his  experience  nobody  could  live  in 
the  camp  longer  than  five  years  without  changing  his  attitudes  so 
radically  that  he  no  longer  could  be  considered  the  same  person  he 
used  to  be.  He  asserted  that  he  did  not  see  any  point  in  continuing 
to  live  once  his  real  life  consisted  in  being  a  prisoner  in  a  concentra- 
tion camp,  that  he  could  not  endure  developing  those  attitudes  and 
behaviors  he  saw  developing  in  all  old  prisoners.  He  therefore  had 
decided  to  commit  suicide  on  the  sixth  anniversary  of  his  being 
brought  into  the  camp.  His  fellow  prisoners  tried  to  watch  him 
carefully  on  this  day,  but  nevertheless  he  succeeded.  [7,  438] 

THE  EGO  MAY  REGRESS,  DISSOCIATE,  OR  BECOME  RE-FORMED 
UNDER  EXTREME  SITUATIONS  OF  STRESS 

Comparatively  few  individuals,  however,  choose  self-destruction 
to  self-modification.  The  more  likely  effect  of  extreme  stress 
caused  by  pressures  from  the  environment  is  some  change  of  the 
ego  formed  during  the  course  of  life.  This  may  be  clear  and 
drastic  or  it  may  be  relatively  hidden  and  subtle.  Numerous 
examples  of  such  alterations  have  been  observed  during  the  past 
few  years  under  the  many  varieties  of  social  upheaval  concomitant 
with  the  rapid  social  change  in  most  Western  societies. 

From  studies  made  on  the  psychological  effects  of  unemploy- 
ment, for  example,  it  can  be  readily  discerned  that  if  an  individual 
is  not  provided  the  opportunity  or  right  to  work,  the  constellation 
of  values  constituting  his  ego  may  deteriorate  radically,  causing  a 
shift  in  his  aspiration  levels  and  a  breakdown  of  former  group 
loyalties.  We  should  emphasize  that  in  this  discussion  we  are  not 


400  BREAKDOWNS  OF  THE  EGO 

generalizing  that  these  are  the  inevitable  effects  of  unemployment 
and  depression  alone,  but  that  unemployment  and  depression 
sometimes  can  and  do  cause  these  effects  as  well  as  others  that  do 
not  concern  us  here.  And  obviously,  how  any  single  individual 
will  react  to  unemployment  will  depend  in  part  upon  personality 
factors  such  as  temperament  and  on  the  length  and  circumstances 
of  unemployment. 

Autobiographies  written  by  unemployed  people  in  Poland  in  the 
early  1930's  and  reported  by  Zawadski  and  Lazarsfeld  [46]  vividly 
reveal  the  "feelings  of  degradation  and  superfluousness"  experi- 
enced. A  youth  writes,  for  example, 

I  become  something  absolutely  superfluous  in  my  own  family.  I  am 
nineteen  years  old,  and  I  define  my  part  quite  clearly  as  that  of  a 
sponger.  [46,  240] 

An  unemployed  man  tells  how  he  no  longer  feels  that  he  is  an 
integral  part  of  society: 

Of  the  many  unemployed  who  have  committed  suicide  lately,  many 
did  it,  as  I  believe,  under  the  influence  of  the  fixed  idea  that  they  had 
become  superfluous  in  the  world.  Really,  every  unemployed  person 
is  excluded  from  the  creative  life.  For  instance  I  had,  as  long  as  I 
worked,  although  I  did  it  for  the  wages,  the  definite  feeling  that  I 
participated  in  the  antlike,  productive  activity  of  mankind.  [46,  240  f.] 

The  effect  of  this  ostracism  on  the  normal  feeling  of  self-respect  is 
clear.  One  of  the  unemployed  writes,  for  example, 

I  look  for  a  job.  /  bow  with  servility,  I  as\,  1  beg,  I  humble  myself 
and  lose  my  ego.  I  become  a  beast,  a  humiliated  beast,  excluded  from 
the  realm  of  society.  [46,  238,  italics  ours] 

Another  says: 

Life  has  made  a  coward  of  me.  Sometimes  I  would  like  to  bend 
myself  in  an  humble  way  before  the  world  and  beg,  "Buy  me!  Buy 
me!"  [46,  238] 

The  effect  of  unemployment  on  one's  own  conception  of  his 
social  status  and  his  loss  of  status  in  the  eyes  of  others  is  exempli- 
fied by  the  statement  of  a  43-year-old  mason  who  wrote: 


EGO  MAY  BECOME  RE-FORMED  UNDER  SITUATIONS  OF  STRESS     401 

How  hard  and  humiliating  it  is  to  bear  the  name  of  an  unemployed 
man.  When  I  go  out,  I  cast  down  my  eyes  because  I  feel  myself 
wholly  inferior.  When  I  go  along  the  street,  it  seems  to  me  that  I 
can't  be  compared  with  an  average  citizen,  that  everybody  is  pointing 
at  me  with  his  finger.  I  instinctively  avoid  meeting  anyone.  Former 
acquaintances  and  friends  of  better  times  are  no  longer  so  cordial. 
They  greet  me  indifferently,  when  we  meet.  They  no  longer  offer  me 
a  cigarette  and  their  eyes  seem  to  say,  "You  are  not  worth  it,  you  don't 
work."  [46,  239] 

After  reviewing  numerous  studies  made  on  the  psychological 
effects  of  unemployment  (1938)  Eisenberg  and  Lazarsfeld  point 
out 

.  .  .  that  the  last  stage  of  unemployment  consists  of  a  general  nar- 
rowing of  activities  as  well  as  of  outlook  on  life.  There  is  also  a 
narrowing  of  wants  and  needs.  Yet  there  is  a  limit  beyond  which  this 
narrowing  cannot  go;  otherwise  a  collapse  occurs.  [12,  378] 

The  effect  of  unemployment  on  the  status  relationship  held  by 
different  family  members  provide  particularly  neat  illustrations  of 
the  way  an  individual's  role  and  position  can  change  if  he  loses  one 
of  his  main  and  traditional  functions,  in  this  case  if  the  father  in 
an  American  family  is  unable  to  carry  out  his  role  as  the  bread- 
winner. Summarizing  the  analysis  of  65  case  studies  collected  in 
Newark  during  the  depression  of  the  early  1930's,  Stouffer  and 
Lazarsfeld,  in  their  monograph  on  The  Family  in  the  Depression 
[41]  state  the  following  "hypotheses"  concerning  the  shifts  in 
family  loyalties  and  authorities: 

1.  If  an  unemployed  father  loses  authority  with  his  wife  he  is  very 
likely  to  lose  authority  with  his  children. 

2.  Conversely,  if  an  unemployed  father  loses  authority  with  his 
children  he  may  or  may  not  lose  authority  with  his  wife. 

3.  If  the  father  is  unemployed,  an  adolescent  daughter  is  more  likely 
to  rebel  against  deprivations  than  an  adolescent  son,  who  gets  unusual 
ego  satisfaction  if  he  can  be  treated  like  a  grownup  as  the  result  of 
even  slight  contributions  to  the  family  budget. 

4.  The  presence  of  an  unemployed  father  in  the  home,  unless  there 
is  great  irritability  and  conflict,  tends  to  increase  his  companionship 
and  authority  with  very  young  children. 


402  BREAKDOWNS  OF  THE  EGO 

5.  If  the  mother  takes  a  job,  after  the  father  loses  his,  authority  over 
the  children  tends  to  be  transferred  to  the  mother. 

6.  The  conflict  between  father  and  son  tends  to  be  greater  if  the  son 
returns  home  unemployed  after  a  period  of  employment  than  if  the 
son  had  never  been  employed. 

7.  If  the  father  is  employed  and  especially  if  he  has  some  influence 
with  his  friends  outside  of  the  family,  a  depression  tends  to  increase 
the  dependence  of  a  son  on  the  father,  since  such  a  father  is  likely  to 
be  the  best  means  of  getting  the  boy  a  job. 

8.  Authority  based  on  categorical  orders  and  punishment  is  more 
likely  to  deteriorate,  in  the  family  of  an  unemployed  father,  than 
authority  based  on  intimacy  and  an  attempt  at  mutual  understanding. 
[41,  116  /.] 

In  Angell's  The  Family  Encounters  the  Depression,  dozens  of 
examples  can  be  found  of  the  restriction  of  the  ego  due  to  unem- 
ployment. The  following  case,  taken  almost  at  random,  describes 
what  happened  to  one  of  the  older  sons  of  a  family  who,  together 
with  his  father  and  sister  who  were  once  wage  earners,  had  lost 
his  job:  [2] 

He  has  completely  lost  his  role.  He  is  unable  to  face  what  he  con- 
siders an  exposure.  He  considers  himself  a  failure.  His  inadequacy 
stands  revealed  to  himself.  He  has  previously  sensed  it,  but  never 
actually  experienced  it.  Without  his  shield  or  a  good  job  he  cannot 
face  the  rest  of  the  world.  Occasionally  he  "talks  big"  but  has  lost  all 
his  braggadocio.  Very  often  he  talks  deprecatingly  of  himself.  Some- 
times he  will  sit  for  hours  and  do  nothing  but  stare.  He  lacks  initia- 
tive of  any  sort.  He  hates  activity  of  any  kind,  hates  to  look  for  work. 

Sometimes  he  will  tirade  against  every  one — the  government,  church, 
home,  friends.  One  feels  a  tension,  a  conflict,  a  strain  when  he  is 
about.  Sometimes  when  this  strain  is  too  great  he  "goes  to  pieces"  in 
a  most  pitiable,  horrible  way.  All  his  inhibitions,  his  restraints,  are 
lifted  and  he  reveals  all  his  hurts,  his  disappointments,  frustrations — 
till  it  is  more  than  others  can  bear.  He  once  became  involved  in  such 
a  scene  in  the  presence  of  several  young  nieces.  All  the  children  were 
weeping  and  utterly  torn  with  sympathy.  He  is  now  incapable  of 
making  the  least  decision  for  himself— for  instance,  to  wear  or  not  to 
wear  a  coat  is  a  source  of  conflict.  [2,  201  /.] 

Many  other  effects  of  unemployment  on  ego  standards  and 
values  can  be  listed  from  a  review  of  the  literature  on  the  subject. 


EGO  MAY  BECOME  RE-FORMED  UNDER  SITUATIONS  OF  STRESS     403 

A  common  finding  is  that  levels  of  aspiration  are  definitely  low- 
ered [26];  some  unemployed  who  never  before  touched  liquor 
may  take  to  drink  [46,  237  /.];  some  may  lose  all  former  respect 
for  those  whom  they  once  regarded  as  their  superiors  and  their 
bosses  [242f.];  some  lose  all  national  loyalties  [249];  and,  if  unem- 
ployed workers  are  not  highly  organized,  class-conscious,  or  in 
some  way  cared  for  by  their  union  or  other  group,  their  solidarity 
with  other  workers  is  apt  to  deteriorate  and  to  be  replaced  by 
feelings  of  hostility  toward  each  other  and  by  individualistic 
competition  and  the  like.  [244  f.] 

In  their  article,  Personality  Under  Social  Catastrophe,  Allport, 
Bruner,  and  Jandorf  summarize  an  analysis  of  90  life  histories  of 
persons  who  experienced  various  degrees  of  persecution  and  suffer- 
ing in  Germany  after  the  Nazi  rise  to  power  on  January  30, 
1933.  [1]  The  authors  state  as  one  of  their  summary  conclusions: 

Even  catastrophic  social  change  does  not  succeed  in  effecting  radical 
transformations  in  personality.  Before  and  after  disaster,  individuals 
are  to  a  large  extent  the  same.  In  spite  of  intensification  of  political 
attitudes  (toward  extreme  opposition)  and  in  spite  of  growing  aware- 
ness and  criticism  of  standards  of  judgment  and  evaluation,  the  basic 
structure  of  personality  persists — and,  with  it,  established  goal  striving, 
fundamental  philosophy  of  life,  skills,  and  expressive  behavior.  When 
there  was  change  in  our  subjects,  it  did  not  seem  to  violate  the  basic 
integrations  of  the  personality  but  rather  to  select  and  reinforce  traits 
already  present.  [1,  19  f.] 

But  they  also  point  out  in  their  conclusion  that,  in  addition  to  the 
stability  of  these  traits: 

Besides  aggressive  responses,  direct  or  displaced,  we  find  defeat  and 
resignation,  regression,  conformity,  adoption  of  temporary  frames  of 
security,  changes  in  standards  of  evaluation,  lowering  of  levels  of 
aspiration,  heightened  in-group  feeling,  increased  fantasy  and  insula- 
tion, and,  above  all,  increased  planning  and  problem-solving.  [1,  20, 
italics  ours] 

Now  just  what  "personality"  is  and  just  what  changes  in  thought 
and  behavior  have  to  take  place  before  personality  can  be  consid- 
ered to  have  "changed"  is  a  problem  we  cannot  go  into  here. 
However,  we  have  already  cited  in  previous  chapters  numerous 


404  BREAKDOWNS  OF  THE  EGO 

instances  of  profound  alterations  in  behavior  and  attitude  due  to 
new  situations  in  which  an  individual  finds  himself.  Without  in 
the  least  denying  the  fact  that  manifestations  of  temperament, 
abilities,  or  organic  characteristics  often  show  an  amazing  con- 
sistency under  different  conditions,  any  conception  of  personality 
which  leaves  out  ego-involvements  seems  quite  inadequate  for 
social  psychology  and,  what  is  more  important,  quite  unsupported 
by  the  facts  of  the  laboratory  and  everyday  life  which  show  so 
clearly  the  effect  of  these  ego-involvements  in  determining  be- 
havior. This  point  of  view  seems  supported  by  the  Allport- 
Bruner-Jandorf  study  itself.  In  commenting  upon  the  changed 
attitudes  toward  National  Socialism  as  the  movement  became 
more  and  more  powerful  and  ubiquitous  in  Germany,  the  authors 
note  "the  inevitable  desertion  of  neutral  attitudes  as  the  social 
crisis  verged  into  personal  catastrophe"  [9],  and  they  quote  from 
one  case  study  the  remark: 

"Everything  was  appraised  according  to  pro-  or  anti-Nazi;  there  was 
simply  no  possibility  of  being  objective.  You  had  to  belong  to  one 
group.  We  individualists  instinctively  felt  the  loneliness  of  our  posi- 
tion outside  any  group."  [1,  9,  italics  ours] 

They  also  point  out  that  "great  anxiety  and  feelings  of  insecurity 
were  practically  universal,"  [11]  and  that  the  most  frequent  cause 
of  this  anxiety  was  "uncertainty  of  future  status."  [12]  Another 
of  their  findings  is  that 

As  the  months  of  persecution  went  on,  42  per  cent  of  the  cases  for 
whom  data  are  available  betrayed  an  increased  fatalism  in  outlook. 
.  .  .  Others  made  attempts  to  come  to  terms  with  the  situation  through 
alterations  in  their  scale  of  values.  ...  In  some  cases,  for  example, 
moral  standards  were  disturbed.  [1,  16,  italics  ours] 

Such  terms  as  "resignation,"  "regression,"  "adoption  of  temporary 
frames  of  security,"  "changes  in  standards  of  evaluation,"  "lower- 
ing of  levels  of  aspiration"  obviously  have  no  meaning  if  the 
implication  that  these  psychological  conditions  are  related  to  the 
ego  is  taken  away. 

Just  what  did  happen  to  the  egos  of  German  citizens  when  they 
were  put  in  very  extreme  situations  has  been  brilliantly  described 
by  Bettelheim  in  his  report  of  his  own  experiences  at  the  German 


EGO  MAY  BECOME  RE-FORMED  UNDER  SITUATIONS  OF  STRESS     405 

concentration  camps  of  Dachau  and  Buchcnwald  and  his  observa- 
tions and  conversations  with  other  inmates.  [7]  Bettelheim  is 
careful  to  point  out  that  "there  were  great  individual  variations'* 
and  that  his  statements  must  be  regarded  as  generalizations. 
However,  it  is  quite  apparent  from  his  account  that  there  were 
limits  to  individual  variations  and  that  generalizations  were,  there- 
fore, quite  possible. 

Summarizing  the  effect  of  the  initial  shock  of  being  taken  pris- 
oner and  deprived  of  civil  rights,  Bettelheim  states: 

It  seems  that  most,  if  not  all,  prisoners  tried  to  react  against  the 
initial  shock  by  mustering  forces  which  might  prove  helpful  in  sup- 
porting their  badly  shaken  self-esteem.  Those  groups  which  found  in 
their  past  life  some  basis  for  the  erection  of  such  a  buttress  to  their 
endangered  egos  seemed  to  succeed.  Members  of  the  lower  class 
derived  a  certain  satisfaction  from  the  absence  of  class  differences 
among  the  prisoners.  Political  prisoners  found  their  importance  as 
politicians  once  more  demonstrated  by  being  imprisoned.  Members 
of  the  upper  class  could  exert  at  least  a  certain  amount  of  leadership 
among  the  middle-class  prisoners.  Members  of  "anointed"  families 
felt  in  prison  as  superior  to  all  other  human  beings  as  they  had  felt 
outside  of  it.  [7,  428  f.] 

The  initial  effort  was,  then,  an  attempt  to  preserve  the  established 
constellation  of  ego-attitudes. 

When  the  prisoners  were  removed  from  the  prison  to  the  con- 
centration camp,  every  effort  was  made  by  the  Gestapo  "to  break 
the  prisoners  as  individuals."  [418]  The  Gestapo  principle  was 
"to  force  the  prisoners  to  feel  and  act  as  a  group,  and  not  as  indi- 
viduals." [434]  In  pursuance  of  this  policy  the  Gestapo  "insisted 
that  none  of  them  [the  prisoners]  was  any  better  than  the  others." 
[447]  Among  other  methods  of  achieving  the  purpose  was 

...  the  hostage  system  and  the  punishment  of  the  whole  group  for 
whatever  a  member  of  it  does;  not  permitting  anybody  to  deviate  in 
his  behavior  from  the  group  norm,  whatever  this  norm  may  be.  [452] 

If  a  prisoner  tried  to  protect  a  group,  he  might  have  been  killed  by 
a  guard,  but  if  his  action  came  to  the  knowledge  of  the  camp  adminis- 
tration then  the  whole  group  was  always  more  severely  punished  than 
it  would  have  been  in  the  first  place.  In  this  way  the  group  came  to 


406  BREAKDOWNS  OF  THE  EGO 

resent  the  actions  of  its  protector  because  it  suffered  under  them.  The 
protector  was  thus  prevented  from  becoming  a  leader,  or  a  martyr, 
around  whom  group  resistance  might  have  been  formed.  [7,  436] 

In  addition  to  various  tortures  administered,  other  methods  of 
destroying  individuality  were  systematic  and  crude  attempts  to  get 
the  individual  to  dissociate  himself  from  established  norms  and 
values.  Prisoners  were  forced 

...  to  hit  one  another,  and  to  defile  what  the  guards  considered  the 
prisoners'  most  cherished  values.  For  instance,  the  prisoners  were 
forced  to  curse  their  God,  to  accuse  themselves  of  vile  actions,  accuse 
their  wives  of  adultery  and  of  prostitution.  This  continued  for  hours 
and  was  repeated  at  various  times.  [7,  429] 

Bettelheim  noticed  that  new  prisoners  who  had  not  yet  been 
broken  or  had  not  resigned  in  any  way  were  unusually  sensitive 
to  any  news  from  the  "outside"  world  which  reflected  any  change 
in  their  social  position. 

Even  the  smallest  change  in  their  former  private  world  attained 
tremendous  importance.  .  .  .  Their  desire  to  return  exactly  the  person 
who  had  left  was  so  great  that  they  feared  any  change,  however  trifling, 
in  the  situation  they  had  left.  Their  worldly  possessions  should  be 
secure  and  untouched,  although  they  were  of  no  use  to  them  at  this 
moment.  [7,  439  f.] 

The  general  feeling  seemed  to  be 

//  nothing  changes  in  the  world  in  which  I  used  to  live,  then  1  shall 
not  change,  either.  .  .  .  The  violent  reaction  against  changes  in  their 
families  was  then  the  counterpart  of  the  realization  that  they  were 
changing.  What  enraged  them  was  probably  not  only  the  fact  of  the 
change,  but  the  change  in  standing  within  the  family  which  it  im- 
plied. [7,  440,  italics  ours] 

Bettelheim  notes  how  particularly  disturbing  and  "unforget- 
table" experiences  were  that  could  not  be  related  to  some  existing 
frame  of  reference. 

It  seems  that  camp  experiences  which  remained  within  the  normal 
frame  of  reference  of  a  prisoner's  life  experience  were  dealt  with  by 
means  of  the  normal  psychological  mechanisms.  Once  the  experience 
transcended  this  frame  of  reference,  the  normal  mechanisms  seemed 


EGO  MAY  BECOME  RE-FORMED  UNDER  SITUATIONS  OF  STRESS     407 

no  longer  able  to  deal  adequately  with  it  and  new  psychological 
mechanisms  were  needed.  [433,  italics  ours] 

The  psychological  reactions  to  events  which  were  somewhat  more 
within  the  sphere  of  the  normally  comprehensible  were  decidedly 
different  from  those  to  extreme  events.  It  seems  that  prisoners  deal 
with  less  extreme  events  in  the  same  way  as  if  they  had  happened 
outside  of  the  camp.  For  example,  if  a  prisoner's  punishment  was  not 
of  an  unusual  kind,  he  seemed  ashamed  of  it,  he  tried  not  to  speak 
about  it,  A  slap  in  one's  face  was  embarrassing,  and  not  to  be  dis- 
cussed. One  hated  individual  guards  who  had  kicked  one,  or  slapped 
one,  or  verbally  abused  one  much  more  than  the  guard  who  really 
had  wounded  one  seriously.  In  the  latter  case  one  eventually  hated 
the  Gestapo  as  such,  but  not  so  much  the  individual  inflicting  the  pun- 
ishment. Obviously  this  differentiation  was  unreasonable,  but  it 
seemed  to  be  inescapable.  One  felt  deeper  and  more  violent  aggres- 
sions against  particular  Gestapo  members  who  had  committed  minor 
vile  acts  than  one  felt  against  those  who  had  acted  in  a  much  more 
terrible  fashion.  [7,  435] 

As  time  went  on  and  as  one  torture  followed  another,  the  author 
consciously  felt  the  pressure  on  his  ego  and  realized  the  inadequacy 
of  established  frames  for  the  interpretation  of  camp  experiences, 
and  the  danger  that  his  body  and  its  daily  life  might  somehow 
become  split  off  or  detached  from  what  he  really  regarded  as 
himself.  He  states: 

If  the  author  should  be  asked  to  sum  up  in  one  sentence  what,  all 
during  the  time  he  spent  in  the  camp,  was  his  main  problem,  he 
would  say:  to  safeguard  his  ego  in  such  a  way,  that,  if  by  any  good 
luc\  he  should  regain  liberty,  he  would  be  approximately  the  same 
person  he  was  when  deprived  of  liberty.  He  has  no  doubt  that  he 
was  able  to  endure  the  transportation,  and  all  that  followed,  because 
right  from  the  beginning  he  became  convinced  that  these  horrible 
and  degrading  experiences  somehow  did  not  happen  to  "him"  as  a 
subject,  but  only  to  "him"  as  an  object.  [7,  431] 

This  observation  was  confirmed  by  others  and  not  limited  to 
Bettelheim's  own  introspection  as  a  psychologist.    He  writes: 

The  importance  of  this  attitude  was  corroborated  by  many  state- 
ments of  other  prisoners,  although  none  would  go  so  far  as  to  state 
definitely  that  an  attitude  of  this  type  was  clearly  developed  already 


408  BREAKDOWNS  OF  THE  EGO 

during  the  time  of  the  transportation.  They  couched  their  feelings 
usually  in  more  general  terms  such  as,  "The  main  problem  is  to 
remain  alive  and  unchanged,"  without  specifying  what  they  meant 
as  unchanged.  From  additional  remarks  it  became  apparent  that 
what  should  remain  unchanged  was  individually  different  and  roughly 
covered  the  person's  general  attitude  and  values.  [7,  431] 

Bettelheim  goes  on  to  describe  this  detachment  he  felt: 

It  was  as  if  he  watched  things  happening  in  which  he  only  vaguely 
participated.  Later  he  learned  that  many  prisoners  had  developed 
this  same  feeling  of  detachment,  as  if  what  happened  really  did  not 
matter  to  oneself.  [431] 

During  the  transportation  the  prisoners  developed  a  state  of  detach- 
ment, feeling  as  if  what  happened  did  not  really  happen  to  them  as 
persons.  [7,  433 1  italics  ours] 

If  an  individual  was  unable  to  keep  his  ego  intact,  was  unable  to 
view  what  was  happening  as  something  not  happening  to  him, 
then  the  only  alternatives  were  to  commit  suicide  or  suffer  a  break- 
down or  dissociation  of  the  ego  and  a  subsequent  re-formation  that 
made  conditions  bearable.  We  have  already  cited  the  case  of  a 
man  who  was  conscious  of  these  alternatives  and  who  chose  sui- 
cide as  the  lesser  of  the  two  evils.  Bettelheim  reports  that  the 
suicides  that  did  occur  were  confined  mainly  to  middle-class 
prisoners  who  were  particularly  anxious 

.  .  .  that  their  status  as  such  should  be  respected  in  some  way. 
What  they  resented  most  was  to  be  treated  "like  ordinary  criminals." 
After  some  time  they  could  not  help  realizing  their  actual  situation. 
Then  they  seemed  to  disintegrate.  [7,  427] 

For  the  majority  of  prisoners  it  seemed  that  this  ability  to  remain 
detached  was  lost,  The  Gestapo  was  successful  in  breaking  down 
individuality.  Old  norms  disappeared,  and  new  ones  took  their 
place.  Bettelheim  reports,  for  example,  that 

Prisoners  who  died  under  tortures  qua  prisoners,  although  martyrs 
to  their  political  conviction,  were  not  considered  martyrs.  Those  who 
suffered  due  to  efforts  to  protect  others  were  accepted  as  martyrs. 
[7,  436] 

And  he  notes  that 


EGO  MAY  BECOME  RE-FORMED  UNDER  SITUATIONS  OF  STRESS      409 

...  as  time  went  on  the  difference  in  the  reaction  to  minor  and 
major  sufferings  slowly  seemed  to  disappear.  [437] 

There  was  a  general  "regression  to  infantile  behavior"  on  the 
part  of  nearly  all  the  prisoners  who  "asserted  their  power  as  a 
group  over  those  prisoners  who  objected  to  deviations  from  normal 
adult  behavior."  [444]  The  prisoners  began  to  live 

.  .  .  only  in  the  immediate  present.  .  .  ,  Friendships  developed  as 
quickly  as  they  broke  up.  Prisoners  would,  like  early  adolescents, 
fight  one  another  tooth  and  nail,  declare  that  they  would  never  even 
look  at  one  another  or  speak  to  one  another,  only  to  become  close 
friends  within  a  few  minutes.  They  were  boastful,  telling  tales  about 
what  they  had  accomplished  in  their  former  lives,  or  how  they  suc- 
ceeded in  cheating  foremen  or  guards,  and  how  they  sabotaged  the 
work.  Like  children  they  felt  not  at  all  set  back  or  ashamed  when  it 
became  known  that  they  had  lied  about  their  prowess.  [7,  445  f.] 

The  relapse  of  more  adult  attitudes  under  the  strain  of  some 
deprivation,  the  stress  of  conditions  that  seem  irreconcilable  with 
established  attitudes,  or  the  internal  conflicts  and  contradictions 
between  various  attitudes  is  a  relatively  common  form  of  ego- 
breakdown.  We  will  note  it  again  in  the  cases  of  war  neuroses 
cited  later  in  this  chapter.  This  regression  to  a  more  infantile  level 
of  adjustment  frees  a  person  from  what  has  become  his  intense 
and  trying  effort  to  maintain  his  more  usual  consistent  status. 
Often  quite  "normal"  people  resort  to  it  temporarily  for  relief. 
Bettelheim  notes  that,  once  this  regression  had  taken  place: 

No  longer  was  there  a  split  between  one  to  whom  things  happened 
and  the  one  who  observed  them  .  .  ,  everything  that  happened  to 
them,  even  the  worst  atrocity,  was  "real"  to  them.  [7,  437] 

The  extent  to  which  the  original  ego  had  broken  down  is  indi- 
cated by  Bettelheim's  statement  that 

Once  this  stage  was  reached  of  taking  everything  that  happened  in 
the  camp  as  "real,"  there  was  every  indication  that  the  prisoners  who 
had  reached  it  were  afraid  of  returning  to  the  outer  world.  [437] 

Old  prisoners  did  not  like  to  mention  their  former  social  status  or 
their  former  activities,  whereas  new  prisoners  were  rather  boastful 
about  them.  [7,  443] 


410  BREAKDOWNS  OF  THE  EGO 

Bettelhcim  reports  that 

Some  of  the  indications  from  which  one  could  learn  about  the 
changed  attitudes  were:  scheming  to  find  oneself  a  better  place  in 
the  camp  rather  than  trying  to  contact  the  outer  world,  avoiding 
speculation  about  one's  family,  or  world  affairs,  concentrating  all 
interest  on  events  taking  place  inside  of  the  camp.  [439} 

Old  prisoners  did  not  like  to  be  reminded  of  their  families  and 
former  friends.  When  they  spoke  about  them,  it  was  in  a  very 
detached  way,  [7,  442] 

It  is  particularly  significant  that  these  "broken"  prisoners  did 
not  become  completely  disorganized  chaotic  individuals.  They 
became,  on  the  contrary,  re-formed  individuals.  Their  repeated 
experience  and  constant  exposure  to  the  values  in  the  microcosm 
of  the  concentration  camp  gave  them  new  frames  of  reference,  new 
anchorages  which  determined  in  large  part  their  thought  and 
behavior  and  with  which  they  identified  themselves.  Bettelheim 
states  that  "a  prisoner  had  reached  the  final  stage  of  adjustment  to 
the  camf  situation  when  he  had  changed  his  personality  so  as  to 
accept  as  his  own  the  values  of  the  Gestapo''  [447,  italics  ours] 
Examples  of  this  identification  with  the  Gestapo's  values  are  cited. 
For  instance, 

Practically  all  prisoners  who  had  spent  a  long  time  in  the  camp 
took  over  the  Gestapo's  attitude  toward  the  so-called  unfit  prisoners. 
...  So  old  prisoners  were  sometimes  instrumental  in  getting  rid  of 
the  unfit,  in  this  way  making  a  feature  of  Gestapo  ideology  a  feature 
of  their  own  behavior.  [7,  448] 

If  a  "traitor"  was  found  in  the  midst  of  the  old  prisoners,  he  was 
slowly  tortured  to  death  just  as  the  prisoners  themselves  were 
tortured  by  the  Gestapo,  instead  of  being  summarily  eliminated. 
The  old  prisoners  tried  to  get  hold  of  and  wear  pieces  of  Gestapo 
uniforms,  tried  to  make  themselves  look  like  the  guards.  Accept- 
ing the  Gestapo  criticism  of  American  and  English  newspaper 
accounts  of  cruelties  committed  in  the  camps,  the  old  prisoners 
"would  insist  that  it  is  not  the  business  of  foreign  correspondents 
or  newspapers  to  bother  with  German  institutions,"  [449] 


EGO  MAY  BECOME  RE-FORMED  UNDER  SITUATIONS  OF  STRESS     411 

When  erecting  buildings  for  the  Gestapo,  controversies  started 
whether  one  should  build  well.  New  prisoners  were  for  sabotaging, 
a  majority  of  the  old  prisoners  for  building  well.  [7,  449] 

The  old  prisoners  took  particular  delight  in  standing  smartly  at 
attention  during  the  daily  roll-call,  prided  themselves  on  being 
tough,  and  even  imitated  in  their  own  games  one  of  the  games 
played  by  the  guards  which  consisted  of  discovering  "who  could 
stand  to  be  hit  longest  without  uttering  a  complaint."  [450]  The 
old  prisoners  accepted  the  Nazi  race  theory;  they  believed  in  the 
need  for  German  Lebensraum.  They  accepted  "as  expression  of 
their  verbal  aggressions,  terms  which  definitely  did  not  originate 
in  their  previous  vocabularies,  but  were  taken  over  from  the  very 
different  vocabulary  of  the  Gestapo."  [447] 

This  study  of  behavior  in  extreme  situations  gives  us,  then,  an 
unusually  vivid  cameo  sketch  of  an  individual's  initial  attempt  to 
defend  and  protect  his  ego,  his  attempt  to  preserve  some  detach- 
ment, his  eventual  regression  to  a  more  primitive  childlike  level 
of  adjustment,  and  the  reconstruction  of  his  ego  along  lines  deter- 
mined by  his  experiences  and  the  prevailing  values  of  those  in 
complete  authority  in  the  little  world  in  which  he  found  himself. 

There  is  no  need  to  spell  out  the  fact  that  the  methods  of 
modern  warfare  create  conditions  for  the  man  in  active  combat 
which  place  on  him  enormous,  sometimes  overwhelming,  strain. 
Fortunately  for  psychology,  the  effects  of  these  harrowing  condi- 
tions on  individuals  were  carefully  observed  by  trained  psychia- 
trists during  World  War  II.  In  their  two  outstanding  books, 
War  Neuroses  and  Men  Under  Stress,  Drs.  Grinker  and  Spiegel 
who  served  with  the  Army  Air  Forces  have  reported  their  work 
in  considerable  detail  and  have  documented  it  with  numerous  case 
studies.  It  is  especially  significant  that  these  two  volumes,  the 
second  of  which  prompted  General  Eisenhower  to  award  the 
authors  the  Legion  of  Merit,  were  both  written  while  the  war  was 
still  going  on,  while  the  authors  were  still  on  active  duty,  not, 
therefore,  constructing  their  impressions  after  a  lapse  of  time  and 
formulating  their  interpretations  in  any  library  or  remote  office. 
[21,  22]  Under  these  conditions  and  with  the  men  they  were 


412  BREAKDOWNS  OF  THE  EGO 

treating  close  at  hand,  the  authors  use  as  their  own  central  theme 
the  effect  of  war-created  situations  on  the  ego.  In  the  preface  of 
War  Neuroses,  they  write: 

Opportunity  is  furnished  by  the  violent,  severe  stress  of  war  to 
observe  the  ego's  capacities  to  handle  the  rapidly  and  intensely  mo- 
bilized biological  and  physiological  drives  signaled  by  anxiety.  [21,  vii] 

Elsewhere  they  state: 

The  appearance  of  the  symptoms  of  a  neurosis  of  war  is  the  signal 
that  the  weakened  ego,  no  longer  able  to  cope  with  the  accumulated 
anxiety,  has  begun  to  sacrifice  a  part  of  its  normal  functions.  [21, 123] 

They  are  careful  to  point  out  that  the  syndromes  they  encountered 
showed  enormous  variety  with  no  two  being  exactly  alike.  Be- 
cause of  this  fact  the  authors  show  a  healthy  disregard  of  any  rigid 
classification  or  pigeonholing. 

In  reviewing  the  accounts  of  Grinker  and  Spiegel,  we  should 
remind  ourselves  that  all  of  the  men  they  treated  were  already 
preselected.  They  had  already  undergone  psychological  and  psy- 
chiatric tests.  And  although  such  tests,  as  the  authors  point  out, 
still  leave  much  to  be  desired,  (since,  for  one  thing,  the  psychia- 
trist has  "no  laboratory  means  of  duplicating  the  stress  to  which  an 
individual  will  be  exposed  in  combat"  [22,  .76]),  still  there  is  little 
doubt  that  the  young  men  selected  for  the  Army  Air  Force  could 
be  regarded  as  above  the  average  in  the  total  population  in  general 
emotional  stability. 

It  should  also  be  noted  that  although  the  publisher's  jacket  for 
War  Neuroses  states  that  the  book  is  "based  on  no  preconceived 
theories,"  the  authors'  treatment  of  the  ego  follows  a  psychoana- 
lytic interpretation  which,  however,  they  do  not  particularly  elabo- 
rate or  defend.  As  they  employ  the  term,  they  do,  however,  fall 
into  the  psychoanalytic  error  of  reifying  the  ego,  endowing  it  with 
an  autonomy  all  its  own.  They  use  such  phrases,  for  example,  as 
"the  ego  interprets,"  [22,  126]  "the  weakened  ego  can  no  longer 
appraise  reality,"  [136]  "the  ego  twists  and  turns  under  the  pres- 
sure." [21, 137]  As  we  point  out  in  chapter  14,  any  such  concep- 
tion of  the  ego  seems  to  us  quite  invalid  and  unnecessary.  But  in 
spite  of  this  loose  use  of  the  term  and  disagreements  we  would 


EGO  MAY  BECOME  RE-FORMED  UNDER  SITUATIONS  OF  STRESS     413 

have  with  many  of  the  authors*  more  detailed  interpretations  of 
specific  cases,  their  rich  and  penetrating  observations  and  their 
placement  of  these  in  an  ego  context  make  their  studies  particu- 
larly pertinent  for  our  purposes  here. 

The  authors  state  that  "one  of  the  most  frequently  heard  remarks 
in  our  wards  was,  'I  took  it  as  long  as  I  could;  I  can't  take  it  any 
more.' "  [21,  69]  In  a  very  real  sense,  this  simple  everyday  state- 
ment of  the  G.I.  sums  up  the  problem  of  ego-breakdown.  The 
job  of  the  psychologist  is,  of  course,  to  determine  precisely  what 
the  "I"  and  the  "it"  mean.  Both  of  these  volumes  contain  elo- 
quent, if  horrible,  testimony  of  the  fact  that  the  "it"  produced  by 
combat  conditions  can  and  often  does  break  down  the  "I." 
Grinker  and  Spiegel  use  the  apt  phrase  "situational  psychotic 
breakdown"  in  describing  the  relationship  between  the  individual 
and  his  immediate  environment.  Just  what  the  authors  mean  by 
"situational  psychotic  breakdown"  is  shown  in  the  following  state- 
ment where  they  point  out  that  under  conditions  of  great  stress 
an  individual  loses  his  intellectual  capacity  to  make  discrimina- 
tions, indulges  in  behavior  not  at  all  characteristic  of  himself,  and 
may  gradually  regress  to  a  less  mature  level  of  adjustment: 

As  the  pressure  on  the  ego  increases,  its  inhibitory  and  intellectual 
functions  begin  to  disappear.  Inhibition  of  the  vegetative  and  motor 
signs  of  anxiety  becomes  impossible.  Try  as  he  may,  the  soldier 
cannot  prevent  the  appearance  of  tremor,  tachycardia,  tachypnea,  and 
restlessness.  This  is  the  situation  in  the  moderate  anxiety  states.  A 
little  more  pressure  of  anxiety,  and  the  intellectual,  appraising  func- 
tions of  the  ego  begin  to  yield.  Thought  becomes  confused  and  con- 
centration impossible.  Discrimination — so  important  in  the  danger 
situation — becomes  lost,  and  when  this  happens  there  occurs  a  further 
increment  to  anxiety,  because  now  an  accurate  evaluation  of  trau- 
matic stimuli  cannot  be  made,  and  all  stimuli  seem  equally  dangerous. 
Caught  in  this  deadly  vortex,  the  ego  twists  and  turns  in  a  desperate 
effort  to  prevent  its  complete  disintegration.  Often  patients— or  sol- 
diers on  the  field — express  this  need  in  the  verbalization:  "I've  got  to 
get  a  grip  on  myself";  and  this  impulse  sometimes  finds  a  motor  outlet 
when  the  patient  clutches  the  nearest  object  tightly  and  squeezes  it  as 
if  his  life  depended  on  it.  As  the  ego  functions  give  way  further, 
various  released  motor  expressions  appear:  uncontrollable  laughing 
and  crying,  aimless  running  about.  Sudden  failures  of  muscular 


414  BREAKDOWNS  OF  THE  EGO 

inncrvation  occur,  comparable  to  the  intellectual  failure;  and  the 
patient  may  fall  down  and  be  unable  to  arise,  because  of  weakness. 
The  loss  of  ego  function  appears  to  be  a  biological  retreat  or  regres- 
sion to  steadily  more  immature  and  primitive  levels  of  ego  develop- 
ment. This  is  not  a  strategic  retreat,  but  a  disorderly  rout,  under  the 
overwhelming  pressure.  [21,  124  f.] 

We  can  include  here  only  portions  from  a  few  of  the  case  studies 
selected  by  Grinker  and  Spiegel  which  provide  the  evidence  for 
this  statement.  The  following  case  of  severe  anxiety  shows  how  a 
man  "lost  control  of  himself  due  to  dissociation: 

A  32  year  old  infantryman  had  taken  part  in  the  severe  fighting  in 
Northern  Tunisia.  Nothing  was  known  of  his  past  history  beyond 
the  fact  that  his  company  had  been  subjected  to  heavy  mortar  fire 
and  dive  bombing,  while  attempting  to  take  a  height  strongly  de- 
fended by  the  enemy.  When  brought  into  the  hospital,  he  was 
unable  to  speak,  and  presented  the  typical  picture  of  severe  terror. 
He  had  coarse,  persistent  tremors  of  the  hands  and  lips  and  started 
violently  when  any  part  of  his  body  was  touched.  At  times  he 
seemed  about  to  speak,  but  nothing  came  of  it  except  inaudible 
whispers.  He  made  no  effort  to  get  out  of  bed  or  to  help  himself  in 
any  way  but  lay  in  a  flexed  posture  with  his  body  curled  up  like  an 
intra-uterine  fetus.  The  diagnosis  on  his  field  tag  was  schizophrenia, 
as  was  the  tentative  diagnosis  made  by  the  admitting  officer.  It  was 
determined  to  give  the  patient  a  few  days'  rest,  sedation,  and  adequate 
nourishment  before  specific  therapy  was  initiated. 

At  the  end  of  two  weeks  the  clinical  picture  had  undergone  con- 
siderable change.  The  patient  was  now  out  of  bed,  but  walked  with  a 
peculiar  simian  gait.  His  knees  were  bent  and  his  shoulders  stooped, 
his  arms  hung  lifelessly  at  his  side  to  below  the  knees,  and  his  head 
and  neck  jutted  forward  at  a  peculiar  angle.  His  facial  expression 
was  one  of  anxious,  puzzled  apprehension.  He  squinted  and  frowned 
at  his  attendants  as  if  trying  to  make  out  who  they  were  and  what  he 
had  to  fear  from  them.  If  an  attendant  made  a  sudden  or  unexpected 
motion,  the  patient  would  start  back  with  fear.  With  much  stam- 
mering, he  asked  simple  questions  in  a  childlike  fashion  about  who  he 
was,  where  he  was,  what  had  happened  to  him.  He  asked  these 
questions  over  and  over  again,  never  receiving  satisfaction.  From 
time  to  time  a  fatuous  smile  would  cross  his  face,  he  would  laugh  and, 
leaping  upon  his  cot,  he  would  jump  up  and  down  on  the  springs 


EGO  MAY  BECOME  RE-FORMED  UNDER  SITUATIONS  OF  STRESS     415 

and  shout  "Dive  bombers!  Dive  bombers  I",  as  if  it  were  a  huge  joke. 
Apparently  an  accomplished  accordion  player  in  the  past,*  he  had  his 
instrument  with  him  and  enjoyed  playing  it.  He  repeatedly  played 
the  song,  "Maybe,"  singing  the  words  to  his  accompaniment  without 
a  trace  of  his  usual  stammer.  When  he  sang,  his  whole  face  lit  up  in 
a  kind  of  ecstasy,  tears  ran  out  of  his  eyes,  and  the  apprehension  dis- 
appeared, only  to  return  as  soon  as  he  put  away  his  instrument. 
There  was  considerable  stereotypy,  and  various  bizarre  mannerisms 
reappeared  in  a  regular  routine. 

After  treatment,  this  patient  made  a  good  recovery.  He  was  able  to 
reconstruct  his  battle  experience,  and  to  orient  himself  in  relation  to 
his  past  life.  Before  his  breakdown  he  had  been  worried  concerning 
his  wife's  pregnancy,  and  it  appeared  that  the  song,  "Maybe,"  was  her 
favorite  melody.  The  bizarre  behavior  disappeared,  and,  although 
much  anxiety  and  depression  in  relation  to  his  battle  experience  re- 
mained, there  was  no  longer  any  question  of  a  diagnosis  of  schizo- 
phrenia. It  was  obvious  that  the  patient  was  in  a  severe  anxiety  state 
with  much  regression  and  disintegration  of  the  ego.  As  is  typical  in 
the  severe  anxiety  states,  recovery  of  the  ego's  functions  proceeded 
rapidly  after  treatment.  For  a  few  weeks  the  gait  continued  to  be 
somewhat  shuffling  and  stooped,  without  the  normal  swinging  of  the 
arms.  The  facial  expression  was  masked,  the  skin  of  the  face  oily, 
and  the  eyelids  blinked  rarely.  A  moderate  tremor  of  the  extremities 
persisted,  which  was  associated  with  rigidity  of  the  cogwheel  type. 
Without  a  knowledge  of  the  past  history  of  this  patient,  one  would 
have  been  tempted  to  diagnose  an  organic  lesion  of  the  extrapyramidal 
system.  After  four  weeks  of  psychotherapy  this  patient  recovered 
from  his  depression,  and  the  facial  expression  became  spontaneous 
and  lively  and  the  gait  normal.  Anxiety  disappeared  except  during 
air  raids.  The  patient  was  able  to  assume  duties  in  the  ward  and 
about  the  hospital,  and  was  eventually  discharged.  [21,  5ff.] 

We  have  already  noted  (pp.  285 /.)  how  individual  men  in  com- 
bat tend  to  identify  themselves  with  their  combat  unit  or  division. 
Grinker  and  Spiegel  particularly  emphasize  the  important  role 
played  by  ego-involving  group  loyalties  in  forestalling  dissociation 
and  regression.  If  the  support  of  this  identification  with  a  group 
is  undermined  either  by  the  loss  in  battle  of  many  group  members 
or  by  the  inability  of  a  group  to  act  as  a  team  under  overwhelming 


416  BREAKDOWNS  OF  THE  EGO 

odds,  then  the  individual  feels  deserted  and  more  alone,  and 
regression  is  much  more  likely. 

Thus,  the  problem  of  anxiety  in  this  context  centers  about  the  posi- 
tion of  the  ego  in  regard  to  mastery,  independence  and  freedom  of 
activity.  In  the  anxiety  problems  of  early  childhood  which  form  the 
core  of  the  usual  neurotic  process,  the  ego  is  peculiarly  helpless  be- 
cause of  its  inexperience  and  its  complete  dependence  on  the  family 
figures  which  constitute  the  threat.  In  combat,  the  ego  is  often 
helpless  because  of  its  position  in  the  group  and  the  nature  of  the 
danger  situation.  The  combat  personality  surrenders  a  great  deal  of 
its  freedom  of  activity  to  the  group,  which  is  then  relied  upon  for 
protection.  The  efficient  functioning  of  the  team  is  the  guarantee  of 
safety,  and  this  applies  equally  to  the  efficient  functioning  of  equip- 
ment, such  as  the  aircraft,  which  must  be  maintained  by  teams. 
As  long  as  the  group  demonstrates  its  ability  to  master  the  dangers 
fairly  effectively,  the  individual  feels  sufficiently  protected  and  com- 
petent  in  the  environment.  But  when  combat  losses  are  high  and 
close  friends  have  been  lost,  or  when  the  individual  has  experienced 
repeated  narrow  escapes  or  a  traumatic  eventf  the  group  is  no  longer 
a  good  security,  and  the  ego  learns  how  helpless  it  is  in  the  situation. 
There  is  no  one  to  rely  upon.  At  the  same  time,  it  cannot  overcome 
the  dangers  by  its  own  efforts  because  of  the  actual  dependence  on 
the  group. 

The  ego  is  thus  in  a  regressed  and  relatively  impotent  position, 
reminiscent  of  childhood.  Its  difficulties  accumulate  as  it  withdraws 
more  affection  from  the  group  and  develops  greater  concern  for  its 
own  fate,  only  to  find  that  this  fate  is  less  and  less  under  control. 
Confidence  in  the  activity  of  the  ego  is  further  shaken  by  the  loss  of 
friends  with  whom  there  is  much  identification,  when  the  thought 
arises:  "He  and  I  were  exactly  alike;  if  it  could  happen  to  him,  it 
can  happen  to  me."  Confidence  is  diminished  in  addition  by  physical 
fatigue  and  illness  and  loss  of  sleep.  A  vicious  circle  is  established, 
leading  to  the  progressive  destruction  of  confidence  in  the  ego's  ability 
to  master  the  dangers.  Out  of  the  ensuing  helplessness  is  born  the 
intense  anxiety.  Thus  severe  anxiety  is  always  secondary  to  regression 
and  dependence  of  the  ego,  and  its  meaning  is  that  in  its  regressed 
position  the  ego  has  been  deserted  by  all  the  forces  which  could  help 
and  protect  it;  nothing  can  be  done,  and  the  only  possible  method 
-of  avoiding  complete  dissolution  is  by  flight.  [22,  129 f.,  italics  ours] 


EGO-BREAKDOWNS  IN  ABNORMAL  AND  PATHOLOGICAL  CASES       417 

These  reports  of  the  effect  of  an  individual's  participation  in  a 
group  under  conditions  imposed  by  war  is  consistent  with  and 
further  confirms  our  discussion  of  ego-involvements  in  group 
situations  in  chapter  10. 

EGO-BREAKDOWNS  IN  ABNORMAL  AND  PATHOLOGICAL  CASES 

The  vast  literature  of  abnormal  psychology  is  filled  with  ex- 
amples of  individuals  who,  for  one  reason  or  another,  change  from 
their  usual  socially  acceptable  and  predictable  selves  to  individuals 
whose  behavior,  perception,  judgment,  thought,  or  imagination  has 
become  disorganized,  so  incompatible  with  reality,  so  dissociated 
or  so  idiosyncratic  that  they  are  no  longer  classifiable  as  "normal." 
Case  studies  reported  by  Janet,  Prince,  Sidis  and  others  have  by 
now  become  classics  in  the  field.  No  matter  what  classification  the 
psychologist  or  the  psychiatrist  may  use  to  describe  a  particular 
patient  (and  these  classifications  do  not  concern  us  here),  the 
general  underlying  symptom  is  that  these  "abnormal"  individuals 
exhibit  in  one  way  or  another  a  distorted  ego  reference,  can  no 
longer  adjust  themselves  to  the  objective  conditions  around  them. 
In  a  short  paper  on  The  Psychopathology  of  the  Ego  System, 
Kisker  and  Knox  [27]  show  how  certain  mental  disorders  can  be 
considered  as  ego  disturbances.  Basing  their  discussions  on 
Koffka's  conception  of  the  ego,  they  note  that  "the  ego  has  of 
necessity  been  postulated  in  psychology  for  the  same  reason  that 
astronomers  were  forced  to  postulate  a  previously  unknown 
planet"  [66  f.]  and  they  believe  that  "a  large  number  of  functional 
mental  disorders  are  matters  of  the  ego  and  its  relationships."  [67] 
Such  mental  disorders  mentioned  are  regression,  "a  detached  ego, 
split  ego,  excessive  development  of  the  'myness'  system,  and  ego- 
isolation."  [68] 

This  breakdown  of  ego  reference  is  vividly  seen  in  what  has 
come  to  be  called  "depersonalization."  In  William  A.  White's 
Outlines  of  Psychiatry  [44],  widely  used  as  a  basic  text  in  the  field 
of  abnormal  psychology,  the  author  sets  the  stage  for  his  discussion 
of  "Disorders  of  Personality"  as  follows: 


418  BREAKDOWNS  OF  THE  EGO 

The  individual,  besides  receiving  certain  information  from  the  en- 
vironment and  forming  certain  ideas,  has  beyond  this  a  consciousness 
of  self,  a  feeling  that  all  his  perceptions  and  ideas  are  experiences  of  a 
single  self,  a  self  that  maintains  its  own  individual  identity  through- 
out, and  which  the  individual  calls  "I."  This  problem  of  self-con- 
sciousness, although  the  riddle  of  psychology,  presents  certain  features 
useful  in  elucidating  the  problem  in  hand.  [44,  69] 

And  in  this  discussion,  after  indicating  what  is  meant  by  "trans- 
formation of  the  personality,"  White  describes  "depersonalization": 

A  lesser  degree  of  the  same  sort  of  process  results  in  a  disorganiza- 
tion, a  breaking  up  of  the  personality.  This  is  seen  in  many  condi- 
tions and  is  associated  with  a  feeling  of  unreality,  and  occurs  as  a 
part  of  the  delirium  of  negation.  The  patients  proclaim  that  they 
are  changed,  they  are  not  themselves.  One  of  my  patients  would  look 
in  the  glass  and  stare  in  wonder  at  her  reflection,  saying  her  eyes 
were  not  hers,  they  were  cat's  eyes.  Another  patient  affirmed  she  had 
no  head,  no  arms,  no  body,  no  mind,  nothing.  The  feeling  of  per- 
sonal identity  in  these  cases  has  become  disrupted,  the  personality 
disorganized.  [44,  70] 

Another  description  of  this  significant  concept  (depersonalization) 
may  be  cited  from  Schilder:  [39] 

It  is  known  that  cases  of  this  type  [depersonalization]  do  not 
experience  other  people  as  personalities,  but  as  inanimate  automatons 
and  dolls.  Furthermore,  the  outward  world  looks  dreamlike  and 
unreal.  Their  own  selves  seem  to  have  disappeared.  The  patients 
feeljhat  they  themselves  act  mechanically.  They  do  not  acknowledge 
their  perceptions,  feelings,  and  emotions.  "There  is  no  real  me. 
I  seem  to  myself  like  a  puppet."  [39,  188  f.] 

Persons  who  are  described  by  the  very  general  label  of  "schizo- 
phrenic" are  characterized  by  the  radical  breakdown  of  the  more 
usual  frames  of  reference  they  had  developed  to  give  meaning  to 
their  environment.  Somehow  these  frames  have  become  inade- 
quate and  inconsistent  When  these  standards  for  judgment  and 
action  collapse,  the  ego  collapses,  because  these  frames  with  all  the 
emotional  overtones  of  value  judgments  they  contained  were  so 
integral  a  part  of  the  self.  A  case  study  by  Paris  [13]  gives  a  good 


EGO-BREAKDOWNS  IN  ABNORMAL  AND  PATHOLOGICAL  CASES      419 

example  of  how  and  probably  why  a  young  man  was  committed 
to  a  state  hospital  with  the  diagnosis  of  schizophrenia.  Paris 
points  out  that 

In  the  course  of  Albert's  life  there  arose  a  number  of  conflicts  and 
problems  which  were  a  great  source  of  torment.  These  tore  at  his 
mind  in  a  variety  of  ways.  The  confused  mores  in  his  culture,  the 
contrasting  groups  to  which  he  held  partial  loyalty,  and  the  incon- 
sistencies of  promise  and  achievement  in  his  career — such  elements 
form  the  contribution  of  social  disorganization  to  the  personal  dis- 
organization he  was  suffering.  His  behavior  during  the  period  of 
his  psychosis  represents  an  adaptation  to  these  problems.  It  was  a 
poor  adaptation,  since  it  did  not  fit  him  to  survive  in  the  competition 
of  the  contemporary  civilization;  but  it  was  organized  and  for  a  time 
seemed  to  promise  better  things.  [13,  135  /.,  italics  ours] 

Among  other  conflicts  Albert  suffered  were  these: 

He  had  acquired  in  his  youth  a  violent  anti-Semitic  prejudice,  only 
to  discover  in  his  adulthood  that  his  family  was  of  Jewish  descent. 
.  .  .  He  had  acquired  a  strong  moral  objection  to  commercial  art, 
which  he  considered  "prostitution"  of  his  talent;  but  he  could  earn 
his  living  only  by  means  of  that  kind  of  art.  .  .  .  During  the  first 
part  of  his  career  his  economic  success  was  satisfactory  and  rapid,  and 
within  five  or  six  years  after  his  start  he  was  earning  as  much  as  $400 
a  month  and  had  plans  for  projects  which  he  hoped  would  carry  him 
further.  His  failures  after  the  collapse  of  the  Florida  boom  and  in 
the  later  major  depression  gave  him  considerable  torment.  [13,  136] 

As  Paris  notes,  the  patient's 

.  .  .  conception  of  his  status  was  derived  not  from  a  harmonious 
social  group  of  which  he  was  a  primary  member  but  rather  from 
distant  and  mixed  sources  and  partly  derived  from  his  reading  and 
daydreaming.  .  .  .  He  could  not  find  a  group  in  which  he  could 
achieve  an  integrated  and  harmonious  personality.  .  .  .  Albert  wast  in 
a  sense,  lost,  since  he  was  culturally  marginal  in  several  respects.  .  .  . 
The  conflicts  produced  in  Albert  internal  strains  and  an  intolerable 
sense  of  being  internally  divided.  .  .  .  The  solution  he  found,  while 
undesirable  from  the  point  of  view  of  survival  in  society,  did  have  a 
logical  relation  to  the  nature  of  Albert's  particular  problems.  It  did 
not  demand  a  change  that  he  could  not  make.  It  resolved  the  conflicts 
so  that  he  could  have  unity  in  his  character  without  pulling  himself 


420  BREAKDOWNS  OF  THE  EGO 

out  of  trouble  by  his  bootstraps.  An  internal  war  thus  came  to  a  sud- 
den end,  and  the  conflicting  elements  temporarily  came  to  rest.  [13, 
136  f.,  italics  ours] 

The  particular  solution  this  patient  adopted  to  relieve  his  marginal 
statul  was  to  identify  himself  with  a  certain  mystical  semireligious 
philosophy  which  the  reading  of  a  certain  book  stimulated  his 
imagination  to  create.  Thereafter  he  acted  as  quite  a  different 
person,  claiming  superhuman  strength,  the  ability  to  stop  any 
speeding  car  or  bullet,  the  ability  to  "be"  any  person  he  desired, 
and  so  on. 

Angyal  describes  another  "schizophrenic"  who  had  lost  all  the 
ego  reference  of  his  experiences,  especially  those  based  on  muscle 
sensation:  [3] 

In  my  patient,  as  in  many  schizophrenic  patients,  the  basic  differ- 
entiation between  the  ego  and  the  external  world  is  grossly  impaired. 
Parts  of  the  patient's  body  and  of  his  mental  and  physical  activities 
are  excluded  from  self-awareness,  arc  not  experienced  as  belonging  to 
his  ego.  The  patient  complains  that  the  jaw  he  has  is  not  his  jaw, 
"it  is  only  set  in."  He  complains  that  his  arm  feels  "lifeless,"  does 
not  belong  to  him;  that  he  has  "no  real  skull  but  only  a  wooden 
frame";  that  his  teeth  are  "false,"  etc.  Some  of  the  thoughts  he  has  are 
not  his  thoughts;  "somebody  gives  them  [to  him]  silently;  they  put 
them  into  [his]  head."  Sometimes  some  words  come  to  him;  he  does 
not  think  them,  "the  words  just  show  up  in  [his]  system."  It  seems 
to  him  also  as  if  some  one  else  thinks  in  him,  "as  if  I  had  two  or  three 
people  with  me."  He  has  also  the  experience  of  having  an  alter  ego 
(Doppel ganger) .  Sometimes  even  his  emotions  are  experienced  as 
disconnected  from  his  ego:  "In  my  stomach  there  is  something,  it 
feels  like  anger."  "In  the  teeth  it  feels  like  anger,  as  if  they  would 
want  to  bite,  to  grab  everything."  Another  time  he  said:  "They  put 
some  glorious  feeling  inside  of  you,  as  if  you  had  done  something 
great."  [1035  f.,  italics  ours] 

In  analyzing  this  particular  case,  Angyal  writes  in  part  as 
follows: 

The  outstanding  feature  in  the  psychopathologic  picture  of  the 
patient  is  profound  disturbances  of  self-awareness.  The  psychologic 
experiences  of  the  patient  have  partly  lost  their  usual  relation  to  the 


EGO-BREAKDOWNS  IN  ABNORMAL  AND  PATHOLOGICAL  CASES      421 

ego:  He  feels  that  parts  of  his  body,  his  thoughts  and  his  feelings  do 
not  belong  to  his  ego  but  are  things  foreign  to  him. 

Analogous  disturbances  are  to  be  observed  in  regard  to  the  psycho- 
motor  activity.  The  patient's  actions  have  partly  lost  their  usual  rela- 
tion to  the  ego.  They  are  excluded  from  the  self  and  appear  to  the 
patient  not  as  his  own  actions  but  as  something  "done"  to  him.  The 
muscular  activities  or  tendencies  to  such  activities  generate  changes 
in  the  muscle  tonus,  which,  because  they  have  lost  their  ego-reference, 
are  psychologically  accounted  for  as  foreign  forces  acting  on  the  body. 
.  .  .  Analysis  of  these  symptoms  clearly  indicates  their  origin  in  mus- 
cular activities  which  have  become  disconnected  from  the  ego.  .  .  . 
The  loss  of  ego-reference  of  muscular  activity  is  a  manifestation  of  a 
more  general  disturbance  of  self -awareness.  The  latterMs  not  further 
analyzed  in  this  paper  but  is  taken  as  a  descriptive  fact.  From  the 
psychologic  point  of  view,  the  loss  of  ego-reference  seems  to  represent 
a  special  \ind  of  defense  mechanism.  [3,  1052 f.f  italics  ours] 

Some  individuals  are  classified  as  "abnormal"  when  they  become 
so  elated  and  euphoric  that  their  behavior  transgresses  the  bound- 
aries of  social  tolerance.  In  such  instances  the  usual  ego-involved 
standards  of  conduct  and  behavior  dissolve.  An  illustration  can 
be  taken  from  McDougall:  [32] 

A  professional  man  in  middle  life,  of  good  heredity,  had  shown  no 
previous  trace  of  instability.  His  history  would  justify  classing  him 
with  the  cyclo-thymic  type.  He  had,  when  young,  suffered  some 
periods  of  very  mild  depression  and  apathy,  such  as  might  be  called 
merely  prolonged  moods  of  discouragement.  And  at  other  times  he 
had  displayed  an  almost  excessive  activity  and  energy,  working  ex- 
tremely hard  in  preparation  for  examinations  and  achieving  athletic 
feats  that  required  tremendous  endurance  and  energy.  He  became 
actively  engaged  in  a  Presidential  campaign.  He  had  long  been 
keenly  interested  in  politics  and  in  certain  planks  of  his  party's  plat- 
form; but  he  had  never  before  taken  an  active  part  in  electioneering, 
whether  State  or  Federal,  and  had  never  spoken  in  public.  He  ap- 
proached his  new  task  with  considerable  diffidence;  but  he  very  soon 
found  that  he  was  an  effective  campaign  orator.  He  was  immensely 
pleased,  stimulated,  and  elated  by  his  success.  He  worked  with  ex- 
treme enthusiasm  and  energy.  He  sought  and  seized  every  oppor- 
tunity for  addressing  public  gatherings.  At  first  his  colleagues  in  the 
particular  local  campaign  were  full  of  admiration;  but  after  some  days 


422  BREAKDOWNS  OF  THE  EGO 

they  were  obliged  to  communicate  with  his  relatives  and  ask  them  to 
remove  him  from  the  scene.  For  his  conduct  had  begun  to  pass  the 
bounds  of  the  normal  and  the  decorous,  and  he  was  beginning  to 
make  himself  a  nuisance  to  them.  He  angrily  resented  all  their  sug- 
gestions to  the  effect  that  he  needed  a  rest  and  had  done  his  share;  he 
was  utterly  impervious  to  their  arguments  and  persuasion.  Instead  of 
taking  a  long  night's  rest  after  his  hard  day's  work,  he  would  get  up 
very  early  in  the  morning  and,  appearing  at  the  window  of  his  hotel 
bedroom,  would  gather  a  crowd  in  the  street  by  his  animated  and 
somewhat  strange  behaviour,  and  deliver  to  them  a  fiery  address, 
freely  exchanging  jokes  and  pleasantries  with  his  auditors.  As  he 
afterwards  put  it,  he  felt  like  a  god;  for  he  could  sway  his  audience 
as  he  wished,  evoking  enthusiastic  agreement  and  applause.  Such 
admiring  response  from  public  gatherings  is,  as  we  know,  strong 
drink  for  any  man.  Even  men  long  and  gradually  accustomed  to 
such  successes  suffer  a  kind  of  intoxication  on  such  occasions;  and,  as 
with  drugs,  they  acquire  a  morbid  need  and  craving  for  ever  new  and 
larger  doses;  they  cannot  live  without  the  "lime-light."  And  in  this 
hitherto  quiet  and  retiring  professional  man  the  intoxication  went  to 
the  point  of  throwing  him  off  his  balance.  He  was  brought  home  by 
the  exercise  of  much  tact  and  patience.  He  refused  to  submit  to 
medical  examination,  declaring  that  he  had  never  before  been  so  fit 
and  strong.  [32,  358  f.] 

Cases  of  split  and  multiple  personalities  are,  perhaps,  the  most 
dramatic  and  well  known  in  the  abnormal  literature.  The  same 
individual  can  be  two  or  more  quite  different  "persons,"  each  may 
have  complete  amnesia  for  the  experiences  of  the  other  and  not 
even  recognize  the  other  self.  Franz's  Persons  One  and  Three  [14] 
can  be  taken  as  fairly  typical.  The  individual  involved  had  ap- 
parently suffered  various  harrowing  experiences  during  World 
War  I  and  was  brought  to  Dr.  Franz's  office  by  the  police  who 
had  picked  him  up  in  a  completely  confused  state  of  mind.  "He 
did  not  recognize  his  surroundings,  he  asked  to  have  his  name 
supplied  to  him,  and  he  did  not  know  the  approximate  date."  [1] 
The  fascinating  story  of  Franz's  successful  effort  to  bring  together 
"persons  One  and  Three"  need  not  concern  us  here.  (Person  Two 
was  represented  by  a  period  of  time  separating  persons  One  and 
Three  and  remained  a  complete  blank  in  the  patient's  conscious- 


EGO-BREAKDOWNS  IN  ABNORMAL  AND  PATHOLOGICAL  CASES      423 

ness.)    What  docs  concern  us  is  that  the  individual  as  person  One 
was  quite  a  different  individual  from  what  he  was  as  person  Three. 

When  asked  about  his  nationality  and  birthplace  he  replied  that  he 
did  not  know  where  he  was  born,  nor  how  old  he  really  was,  although 
he  could  approximate  his  age  within  a  few  years.  He  had  no  idea 
what  his  father's  occupation  was,  nor  did  he  know  what  he  himself 
had  been  doing  prior  to  April,  1915,  at  which  time  he  became  con- 
scious of  himself.  [14,  26] 

Even  self-recognition  was  lacking: 

Later  I  led  him  into  another  room  in  which  there  was  a  mirror,  and 
I  suggested  that  he  look  at  himself  in  the  glass.  He  appeared  to  be 
greatly  upset  at  his  appearance,  and  after  having  taken  one  look,  he 
said:  "My  God,  is  that  me?"  [14,  62] 

Habit  systems  were  altered,  as  the  following  indicates: 

Twice  also  Jack  was  found  to  have  purchased  cigars,  and  to  have 
smoked  them.  In  his  so-called  "normal"  state  he  did  not  smoke  cigars. 
He  had  not  smoked  them  since  1914.  When,  therefore,  Jack  re- 
verted to  his  first  personality  the  old  habit  came  to  the  fore.  [14,  131] 

Franz's  description  of  the  first  meeting  of  persons  One  and 
Three  is  revealing.  While  the  doctor  was  going  over  a  map  with 
the  patient  in  an  attempt  to  help  him  reconstruct  the  course  of  his 
wanderings  as  a  soldier  in  World  War  I: 

Suddenly  he  stopped,  and  with  his  finger  directed  to  a  point  and  a 
name,  said,  "Voi!  I  was  there.  I  had  a  monkey."  His  emotion  was 
obvious.  He  was  greatly  excited.  Memory  was  trickling  through. 
The  walled-off  memories  could  not  be  kept  impounded.  The  face  of 
the  dam  gave  way,  and  there  was  an  onrush  carrying  all  before  it. 
It  was  irresistible.  The  memories  were  eddying,  and  tossing  one  over 
another.  His  remarks  came  too  rapidly  to  be  recorded.  He  traveled 
from  Africa  to  France,  to  the  United  States,  to  England,  and  to  Ire- 
land, back  and  forth.  Ideas  were  tumbling  about  in  their  mad  rush 
to  come  to  the  surface.  In  a  few  minutes  he  had  lived  fifteen  years. 
He  had  met,  and  he  had  recognized,  himself.  [14,  79,  italics  ours] 

Although  Franz  was  unable  to  learn  much  of  the  early  history  of 
his  strange  patient,  he  writes  at  the  end  of  his  account  that 


424  BREAKDOWNS  OF  THE  EGO 

...  it  is  worthy  of  note  that  every  change  which  had  been  ob- 
served, or  of  which  we  have  had  reasonable  information,  was  known 
to  be  preceded  by  a  period  in  which  he  was  under  what  is  commonly 
called  "emotional  strain."  He  was  worried  financially,  or  he  was 
irritated,  or  he  was  fearful,  or  he  may  have  been  too  happy.  [14, 188] 

An  example  of  several  selves  living  together  simultaneously  in 
the  same  person  is  seen  from  the  following  account  which  a 
schizophrenic  wrote  of  himself: 6 

I  am  a  peculiar  person.  In  fact  I  do  not  know  rather  [whether] 
you  would  call  me  a  person  or  not.  I  did  possess  four  (4)  bodies  that 
were  me.  Unlike  the  twin  Severins  I  do  lack  the  middle  brain  they 
possess.  I  do  not  know  the  origin  of  my  specie  or  of  any  other  specie. 
I  do  know  people  call  our  specie  Gouls.  I  have  lined  [linked?]  eyes; 
that  is,  when  the  four  of  me  took  hands  we  could  see  in  four  direc- 
tions at  once.  I  have  heard  the  Twin  Severins  lack  duplicate  eyes  and 
bodies.  I  did  possess  thought  transmission.  As  far  as  my  early  life 
is  concerned  I  do  not  recall  very  much;  but  about  three  years  I  can 
remember  all  of  my  other  segiments.  When  I  was  about  6  years  old 
I  lost  three  of  my  segiments  and  received  a  braining  at  the  same  in- 
stant. I  do  not  imagine  any  of  you  people  can  understand  the  mixure 
of  being  in  different  bodies  and  places  at  the  same  time.  I  have  for- 
gotten all  about  not  being  like  other  people. 

After  a  very  careful  review  of  the  "psychological  deficits"  of 
psychotics,  Hunt  and  Gofer  [25]  conclude  that  "we  should  like  to 
conceive  this  deficit  in  capacity  in  terms  of  a  rigidity  of  set  and  a 
reduction  in  the  number  of  tasks  or  lines  of  activity  that  can  be 
sustained  simultaneously  or  synthesized  into  a  single  complex 
operation."  [1014] 

The  deficit  in  the  "functional"  psychoses,  and  particularly  in  schizo- 
phrenia, we  conceive  as  an  extinction  of  standards  for  performance 
and  of  thought  skills  that  have  been  socially  rewarded.  [25,  1022, 
italics  ours] 

This  conclusion  is  entirely  in  line  with  our  formulation:  "set," 
"standards  for  performance,"  and  "thought  skills,"  are  familiar  to 

6  This  is  taken  from  one  of  a  number  of  essays  Professor  Wendell  Johnson  of 
the  University  of  Iowa  has  collected  from  institutionalized  people  and  which  he 
has  kindly  let  us  examine. 


EGO-BREAKDOWNS  IN  ABNORMAL  AND  PATHOLOGICAL  CASES       425 

us  as  anchorages,  standards  of  judgment,  frames  of  reference,  or 
attitudes  which  have  become  ego-involved  and  whose  "extinction" 
is  therefore  bound  to  bring  about  the  extinction  of  the  normally 
functioning  ego. 

Organic  disturbances.  So  far  in  our  discussion  of  abnormal 
cases,  we  have  considered  instances  commonly  labeled  as  functional 
rather  than  as  organic.  With  the  rapid  development  of  neurology 
toward  the  end  of  the  19th  century,  psychologists  began  to  look 
for  the  psychological  effects  of  organic  disturbances  in  the  brain. 
In  this  effort,  Ribot  was  one  of  the  pioneers.  Stimulated  by 
Charcot's  work  in  psychopathology,  Ribot  [36,  37]  brought  to 
his  work  the  physiological  knowledge  of  his  day.  Although  the 
neurology  of  Ribot's  time  seems  to  us  now  somewhat  crude,  and 
although  Ribot  tended  to  regard  all  psychoses  as  organic,  he  was 
among  the  first  to  point  out  that  disorders  of  brain  functions  could 
cause  a  "dissolution  of  the  personality"  and  a  disintegration  of  the 
unity  of  the  ego  (Le  Moi).  The  ego  was  central  in  Ribot's  formu- 
lations. He  regarded  it  as  a  "sum  of  conscious  states"  subjectively 
known  from  coenaesthesis  and  from  memory. 

"An  organic  lesion,"  he  wrote  in  1881,  "may  transform  the 
coenaesthesis,  substituting  for  the  normal  sensation  of  existence  a 
condition  of  melancholy,  mental  distress,  and  anxiety,  of  which 
the  patient  is  unable  to  discern  the  cause;  or,  on  the  other  hand, 
producing  undue  joyousness,  exuberant  emotions,  and  extreme 
content."  [36, 109]  Anything  that  affects  the  vital  processes  upon 
which  "the  sum  of  actual  states  of  consciousness"  depends  will 
necessarily  affect  the  ego. 

Feelings  form  the  self;  amnesia  of  the  feelings  is  the  destruction  of 
the  self.  It  is  logical,  then,  that  a  period  should  arrive  when  dis- 
organization becomes  so  great  as  to  disintegrate  personality.  [36,  121] 

Ribot  pointed  out  that  gross  injuries  to  the  brain  could  and  did 
cause  disturbances  and  deteriorations  of  the  higher  more  abstract 
mental  processes  (ideas  and  memories),  and  in  this  process  of  de- 
terioration he  observed  that  it  was  the  more  recently  acquired  most 
highly  developed  intellectual  abilities  that  tended  to  disintegrate 
first  with  the  adjustments  and  memories  of  childhood  "the  last  to 
disappear." 


426  BREAKDOWNS  OF  THE  EGO 

With  the  increased  precision  of  our  knowledge  of  physiology 
and  with  the  development  of  more  refined  psychological  tests,  the 
effects  of  more  or  less  localizable  lesions  have  become  amenable 
to  more  rigorous  investigation.  There  is  no  need  to  review  this 
work  here  in  detail.7  The  outstanding  work  of  Gelb  and  Gold- 
stein is  especially  relevant  to  our  problem  of  ego-breakdowns.  [17] 

From  their  tests  of  color  discrimination  among  patients  who  had 
suffered  lesions  in  the  frontal  region  as  the  result  of  war  injuries, 
Gelb  and  Goldstein  concluded  that  there  was  a  very  definite 
impairment  of  the  ability  to  generalize  or  to  make  abstractions. 
The  patients  tended  to  be  limited  to  what  the  authors  described 
as  a  "concrete  attitude."  This  effect  of  organic  brain  injuries  on 
abstract  thinking  has  been  confirmed  by  Weigl  [43],  Ry lander 
[38],  Halstead  [23]  and  others.  Goldstein  and  Scheerer  have 
more  recently  reported  the  results  of  further  tests  devised  to  study 
this  loss  of  the  "abstract  attitude."  [20]  Without  taking  sides  one 
way  or  another  in  the  controversy  concerning  Goldstein's  termi- 
nology, it  is  significant  for  us  to  notice  that  in  the  description  of 
the  "abstract  attitude,"  Goldstein  and  Scheerer  place  as  the  first 
characteristic  in  their  list  the  loss  of  ego  reference.  All  of  the  other 
characteristics  they  describe  refer  in  one  way  or  another  to  the 
deterioration  of  normal  frames.  They  state: 

The  abstract  attitude  is  the  basis  for  the  following  conscious  and 
volitional  modes  of  behavior: 

1.  To  detach  our  ego  from  the  outer  world  or  from  inner  experi- 
ences. 

2.  To  assume  a  mental  set. 

3.  To  account  for  acts  to  oneself;  to  verbalize  the  account. 

4.  To  shift  reflectively  from  one  aspect  of  the  situation  to  another. 

5.  To  hold  in  mind  simultaneously  various  aspects. 

6.  To  grasp  the  essential  of  a  given  whole;  to  break  up  a  given 
whole  into  parts,  to  isolate  and  to  synthesize  them. 

7.  To  abstract  common  properties  reflectively;  to  form  hierarchic 
concepts, 

8.  To  plan  ahead  ideationally;  to  assume  an  attitude  towards  the 
"mere  possible"  and  to  think  or  perform  symbolically.  [20,  4] 

7  Thorough  reviews  of  the  literature  on  the  psychological  effects  of  brain  lesions 
will  be  found  in  [9]  and  [28]. 


EGO-BREAKDOWNS  IN  ABNORMAL  AND  PATHOLOGICAL  CASES       427 

The  illustrations  used  by  Goldstein  and  Scheerer  to  exemplify 
these  various  characteristics  of  the  abstract  attitude  which  is  lost 
by  those  with  a  cerebral  pathology  are  so  illuminating  that  we 
shall  quote  here  in  full  the  examples  given  of  the  deterioration  of 
the  first  three  modes  of  behavior  previously  mentioned. 

1.  To  detach  our  ego  from  the  outerworld  or  from  inner  experiences. 

Patient  F.  is  asked  to  take  a  comb  from  a  table  and  bring  it  to  the 
examiner.  She  cannot  do  this  without  combing  her  hair  ("forced  re- 
sponsiveness").  A  patient  of  Head  says,  "With  me  it's  all  in  bits,  I 
have  to  jump  like  a  man  who  jumps  from  one  thing  to  the  next;  I 
can  see  them,  but  I  can't  express  them."  Patient  Sch.  is  asked  to  re- 
peat the  sentence:  "The  snow  is  black."  He  states  he  could  not  say  it, 
that  it  was  not  so.  The  examiner  explained  to  him  that  such  sense- 
less phrases  can  be  repeated  even  though  they  are  not  true,  and  then 
urged  the  patient  to  repeat  the  sentence.  Now  the  patient  repeated 
the  requested  sentence,  but  mumbled  immediately  afterwards:  "No, 
the  snow  is  white."  The  same  patient  could  not  be  induced  to  re- 
peat the  sentence  "the  sun  is  shining"  on  a  rainy  day.  Patient  Schor. 
with  a  paralysis  of  the  right  arm  was  unable  to  repeat  the  sentence  "I 
can  write  well  with  my  right  hand."  He  replaced  the  word  "right" 
by  the  word  "left."  Another  patient  was  well  able  to  use  eating  uten- 
sils while  eating,  whereas  given  these  objects  outside  of  the  eating 
situation,  he  produced  only  a  jumble  of  senseless  movements  with 
them.  Another  patient  was  unable  to  drink  water  out  of  a  glass  on 
command,  unless  he  was  really  thirsty. 

2.  To  assume  a  mental  set  willfully  and  consciously. 

A  patient  is  unable  to  set  the  hands  of  a  clock  to  the  demanded 
hour,  but  can  recognize  what  time  it  is  immediately  if  presented  with 
the  clock.  Another  patient,  whom  the  examiner  started  off  on  a 
continuous  task,  e.g.,  counting  or  writing  letters,  is  unable  to  proceed 
spontaneously  if  once  interrupted;  he  is  unable  to  initiate  an  action 
on  his  own,  to  assume  a  mental  set  willfully.  A  patient  of  Woerkom 
could  give  the  series  of  the  week  days  and  months  of  the  year  cor- 
rectly, but  if  the  examiner  named  a  particular  day  or  month,  the 
patient  could  not  give  the  name  of  the  one  preceding  or  following. 

3.  To  account  for  acts  to  oneself  or  to  others. 

A  patient  is  well  able  to  throw  balls  into  three  boxes  which  are 
located  at  different  distances  from  him.  He  never  misses.  Asked 


428  BREAKDOWNS  OF  THE  EGO 

which  box  is  further  and  which  is  nearer,  he  is  unable  to  give  any 
account  or  to  make  a  statement  concerning  his  procedure  in  aiming. 
Another  patient  points  correctly  to  the  source  of  a  noise,  but  cannot 
state  the  direction  from  which  the  noise  originated.  To  do  this  re- 
quires an  abstract  grasp  of  spatial  relation  and  the  concomitant  capac- 
ity to  account  for  this  understanding  by  verbalization.  [20,  4  /.] 

Their  characterization  of  the  "concrete"  attitude  indicates  the 
central  position  of  this  loss  of  ego  reference. 

We  surrender  to  experiences  of  an  unreflected  character:  we  are 
confined  to  the  immediate  apprehension  of  the  given  thing  or  situa- 
tion in  its  particular  uniqueness.  .  .  .  We  surrender  to  the  immediate 
claims  and  particular  uniqueness  of  thought  and  feelings  in  the  same 
way  as  to  the  outer  world  claims.  [2/.,  italics  ours] 

Goldstein  and  Scheerer  are  careful  to  point  out  that  the  abstract 
attitude  must  be  regarded  as  a  relatively  recent  evolutionary  de- 
velopment, as  a  "new  emergent  quality."  [22]  And  that  Goldstein 
is  not  using  the  concept  of  the  ego  in  any  loose  psychoanalytic 
sense  as  a  mysterious  autonomous  entity  is  indicated  in  another  of 
his  publications  [18]  in  which  he  writes,  in  a  discussion  of  repres- 
sion: 

The  factor,  which  actuates  the  so-called  repressing,  is  formed  neither 
through  prohibitions  from  without,  nor  by  a  censor,  nor  by  an  ego, 
nor  by  a  super-ego.  Rather,  through  maturation,  new  patterns  of  the 
organism  are  formed,  conforming  to  the  human  species  in  general 
and  to  the  cultural  pattern  of  that  particular  milieu  in  which  the  child 
grows  up.  Of  course,  one  can  call  this  development  "ego-formation" 
and  of  course,  the  prohibitions,  just  li\e  other  processes  in  the  environ- 
ment, are  co-determining  factors  in  this  formation.  [18,  320,  italics 
ours] 

A  revealing  illustration  of  the  effect  of  a  temporary  concussion 
on  the  ego  is  given  by  Koffka  when  he  cites  the  report  of  a  moun- 
tain climber  who  had  lost  consciousness  after  falling  into  a  chasm. 
[29]  The  climber  reported  his  "awakening"  as  follows  (Koffka's 
literal  translation): 

".  .  .  fog  .  ,  .  darkness  ...  fog  ...  whirring  .  .  .  grey  veil  with 
a  small  lighter  spot  .  .  .  fog  .  .  .  faint  dawn  ...  a  soft  humming 
.  .  .  dull  discomfort  .  .  .  fog  .  .  .  something  has  happened  to  some- 


EGO-BREAKDOWNS  IN  ABNORMAL  AND  PATHOLOGICAL  CASES       429 

body  .  .  .  gloomy  fog,  and  always  that  lighter  point  ...  a  shivering 
shudder:  something  clammy  .  .  .  fog  .  .  .  how  was  it?  ...  an  ef- 
fort at  thinking  ...  ah,  still  fog;  but  besides  that  light  point  there 
outside,  there  emerges  a  second  point  inside :  right,  that  is  //  .  .  .  fog, 
dull  ringing  sound,  frost  ...  a  dream?  .  .  .  Yes,  indeed,  a  wild, 
wild,  wild  dream! — It  has  dreamed — no,  rather  /  have  dreamed.  .  .  ." 
[29,  323  /.] 

As  Koflfka  remarks: 

Phenomenally  quite  a  long  period  of  time  passed  without  an  Ego. 
.  .  .  The  Ego  did  not  even  emerge  with  the  first  articulation  of  the 
field,  the  light  point,  not  even  with  the  first  feeling  of  discomfort,  and 
apparently  not  even  with  the  first  conscious  thought,  though  it  was 
this  which  led  very  soon  to  the  momentary  establishment  of  the  Self, 
which  was,  however,  as  yet  quite  unstable.  [29,  324] 

In  his  summary  of  the  effects  of  brain  lesions  on  personality, 
Stanley  Cobb  [9]  indicates  that  severe  concussions  or  contusions 
of  the  brain,  while  they  have  comparatively  little  effect  on  the  "in- 
tellectual" functions  such  as  memory,  are  very  likely  to  extinguish 
those  personal  and  social  standards  of  judgment  which  determine 
an  individual's  moral  codes. 

Persons  who  have  had  several  concussions  or  contusions  of  the  brain 
often  develop  a  clinical  picture  that  is  quite  characteristic:  after  each 
injury,  but  usually  more  conspicuously  after  the  third  or  fourth,  the 
patient  has  persistent  headache,  increased  by  using  the  eyes  or  expo- 
sure to  the  sun;  transient  diplopia  is  common.  He  avoids  lifting  or 
leaning  over  because  of  increased  headache  and  dizzy  feelings.  After 
a  few  months  this  state  becomes  one  of  general  invalidism  with  many 
aches,  easy  fatigability,  lack  of  initiative,  and  varying  degrees  of 
apathy.  Close  psychiatric  supervision  may  keep  him  socially  ac- 
ceptable, but  he  usually  drifts  off,  becomes  a  "rolling  stone"  and 
"irresponsible."  Alcohol  affects  him  strongly  and  may  precipitate 
epileptic  attacks  of  various  kinds.  Psychological  examination  shows 
little  intellectual  deficit  in  such  fields  as  memory,  retention,  and  alert- 
ness, but  as  the  process  goes  on,  such  symptoms  of  deterioration  may 
appear.  During  the  first  year  of  his  disorder  the  patient  seems  to  be 
simply  unreliable;  later  he  becomes  incorrigible,  a  pathological  liar, 
always  taking  the  easy  way  out  and  avoiding  sustained  effort.  Some 


430  BREAKDOWNS  OF  THE  EGO 

of  these  patients  are  difficult  to  distinguish  from  the  "ne'er  do  well* 
and  chronic  delinquent  diagnosed  "psychopathic  personality  and 
"moral  imbecile!'  [9,  557,  italics  ours] 

In  this  same  discussion,  Cobb  also  reports  the  changed  person- 
ality brought  about  by  encephalitis  (an  "inflammatory  reaction 
within  the  brain  due  to  invasion  by  micro-organisms").  [559] 

The  patient  may  come  first  to  the  physician  complaining  of  fatigue 
and  nervousness.  .  .  .  The  family  will  complain  that  the  patient  is 
becoming  coarse  and  ill-mannered,  irritable  and  forgetful.  The  jovial 
megalomania  so  often  described  may  be  present,  but  it  is  not  the  rule; 
a  mood  of  depression  is  not  uncommon  instead  of  the  textbook  eu- 
phoria. But  when  the  elated,  expansive  mood  is  present,  it  is  a  most 
remarkable  phenomenon:  one  sees  a  man  in  the  prime  of  life,  who 
has  previously  been  modest  and  reliable,  rather  suddenly  break  forth 
with  ideas  of  omnipotence  and  actions  dishonest  and  spectacular. 
The  usual  picture,  however,  is  less  striking,  with  emphasis  on  the 
gradual  loss  of  culture  and  memory. 

I  remember  well,  when  acting  as  clinical  clerk  at  Queen  Square  for 
Kinnier  Wilson,  I  saw  him  present  a  new  case  to  a  group  of  students 
in  the  out-patient  clinic.  I  was  seated  at  the  table  taking  notes,  Wilson 
was  standing,  having  just  dismissed  a  patient,  and  there  was  an  empty 
chair  beside  my  table.  Wilson  rang  for  the  next  patient,  the  door 
opened  and  a  man  entered,  followed  by  his  wife.  He  walked  across 
the  fifteen  feet  of  classroom,  smiled  at  the  students  and  at  me,  and  sat 
down.  Wilson  turned  to  me  instantly  and  said,  "Write  down  G.P.I. 
as  the  diagnosis."  Probably  my  jaw  dropped,  for  he  went  on,  "Well, 
Cobb,  what  else  could  it  be?  Here  is  a  middle-aged  man  coming  to 
a  nerve  clinic.  He  enters  the  room  smiling,  pushes  ahead  of  his  wife, 
does  not  take  off  his  hat,  takes  the  only  chair  without  asking  and  likes 
an  audience!"  Subsequent  neurological  and  serological  studies  proved 
the  correctness  of  the  diagnosis.  [9,  559  /.] 

Cobb  remarks  that  the  striking  thing  about  those  suffering  from 
general  paresis  is  their  loss  of  "good  manners  and  culture"  at  the 
outset  of  the  disease.  [560] 

We  saw  in  earlier  chapters  how  the  ego  develops  in  childhood 
and  adolescence.  We  can  see  in  cases  of  senility  how  the  ego 
breaks  down  and  deteriorates  in  old  age.  Muncie  describes  senility 
in  his  chapter  dealing  with  "the  acquired  organic  deficit  reactions" 
[34,  ch.  10],  and  Kraines  indicates  that 


EGO-BREAKDOWNS  IN  ABNORMAL  AND  PATHOLOGICAL  CASES       431 

The  essential  pathology  of  senile  dementia  is  an  abiotrophy  of  the 
ganglion  cells  of  the  cerebral  cortex.  [30,  382] 

The  senile  reaction 

...  is  a  gross  caricature  of  the  normal  aging  process  with  its 
circumscription  of  interests,  the  backward  glance  at  things  long  past, 
and  the  garrulous  reminiscences.  [34,  409,  italics  ours] 

Muncie  describes  how  the  "frontier  of  memory"  is  "pushed  steadily 
backward  with  the  progress  of  the  disease."  [409] 

Closely  related  to  this  disturbance  of  intellectual  functions  are 
changes  in  personality  which  "are  extremely  common"  according 
to  Kraines.  Among  these  changes  are  increasing  irritability,  out- 
bursts of  anger  and  resentment,  increasing  egotism,  acute  excite- 
ment or  depression.  Commenting  on  a  senile  patient  whose  case 
he  describes,  Muncie  summarizes  as  follows: 

A  simple  decline  in  the  assets,  especially  those  of  memory  and 
judgment,  living  in  the  remote  past,  and  disoriented  for  the  present. 
The  speech  showed  loss  in  the  reverse  order  of  its  acquisition  and 
finally  was  reduced  to  unintelligible  jargon.  Emotional  lability,  with 
fairly  good  social  preservation;  evasion  and  confabulation  were 
marked  features.  [34,  413] 

This  "second  childhood"  characteristic  of  so  many  senile  patients 
clearly  shows  the  regression  to  a  relatively  simple  narrow  level  of 
ego  functioning,  the  disintegration  of  the  ego-attitudes  of  the  more 
normal  adult. 

How  the  ego  is  affected  by  surgical  insult  to  the  frontal  lobes. 
Brickner's  well-known  study  of  the  effect  of  the  removal  of  part  of 
both  the  frontal  lobes  of  a  New  York  stockbroker  (referred  to  as 
A  in  the  following  quotations)  shows  beyond  the  shadow  of  a 
doubt  that  the  loss  of  these  higher  areas  of  the  brain  also  brings 
about  a  loss  of  ego-involved  standards  for  behavior  and  thought. 
A  mere  listing  of  some  of  the  title  headings  of  chapters  that  con- 
tain detailed  recorded  conversations  with  the  patient  indicates  the 
generalizations  derived  from  hours  of  close  observation:  [8] 

Impairment  of  Restraint  in  Controlling  or  Concealing  Emotion. 
Impairment  of  Restraint  as  Indicated  by  Boasting  or  Self-Aggran- 
dizement. 


432  BREAKDOWNS  OF  THE  EGO 

Impairment  of  Restraint  as  Indicated  by  Reminiscing  Which  Is 

Utilized  for  Self-Aggrandizement. 
Impairment  of  Restraint  as  Indicated  by  A's  Free  Expression  of 

Mild  Hostility. 
Impairment  of  Restraint  as  Indicated  by  the  Free  Expression  of 

Angry,  Aggressive,  Negativistic,  Puerile  Impulses  Employed  for 

Superarrogation,  Sometimes  Combined  with  Hostility.  [8,  xiiif.] 

Brickner  concludes  from  his  detailed  analysis  that 

The  most  prominent  symptomatic  mark  of  A's  conduct  is  the 
relatively  free  exhibition  of  emotional  and  instinctive  phenomena. 
The  three  constantly  recurring  themes  of  A's  action  are  self-aggran- 
dizement, hatred  and  sexuality. 

A's  exaggerated  need  for  ego  aggrandizement  is  easy  to  perceive. 
[275] 

There  is  nothing  about  A's  impairment  of  restraint  which  is  not  to 
be  found  in  any  young  child  who  has  not  yet  learned  enough  about 
the  requisite  appreciations  to  make  choices  and  to  plan  his  behavior. 
[8,  280] 

And  he  notes  that  "such  highly  derived  attainments  as  'moral 
sense/  'social  sense/  'character'  and  'personality'"  are  dependent 
on  "judgment,  critique,  abstractive  ability,  generalizing  capacity, 
appreciation  of  situations,  sense  of  humor  and  others."  And  it  is 
apparently  these  psychological  capacities  that  were  "removed" 
along  with  the  sections  of  A's  frontal  lobes.  The  author  notes  that 
many  earlier  writers  on  the  subject  have  used  the  term  "inhibition" 
to  describe  the  psychological  consequences  of  frontal  lobe  symp- 
toms. Among  these  earlier  writers  were  Elder  and  Miles  (1902) 
who  stated  that  frontal  lobe  symptoms 

...  are  exceedingly  difficult  to  analyze  and  are  even  more  difficult 
to  describe.  The  absence  of  such  symptoms  renders  a  man  what  he 
is  ...  [their]  presence  .  .  .  alters  the  ego  of  a  man,  takes  away  his 
special  mental  characteristics  and  reduces  him  to  a  lower  level.  [8,  276] 

Freeman  and  Watts  have  used  frontal  lobotomy  as  an  admittedly 
last  resort  cure  for  what  they  describe  as  "overwhelming  conscious- 
ness of  the  self."  [15]  Summarizing  the  results  of  66  such  opera- 
tions, they  write: 


EGO-BREAKDOWNS  IN  ABNORMAL  AND  PATHOLOGICAL  CASES       433 

Prefrontal  lobotomy  bleaches  the  affective  component  connected 
with  the  consciousness  of  the  self.  Patients  who  are  afflicted  with  a 
distressing  consciousness  of  the  self  (either  as  a  unit  of  society  or  as  a 
collection  of  organs)  and  whose  preoccupation  is  fixed  and  unyielding, 
will  secure  from  prefrontal  lobotomy  definite  relief  even  though  cer- 
tain of  their  ideas  and  actions  may  persist  for  a  long  time.  Reduction 
of  the  affective  component  allows  the  personality  to  appear  in  purer 
form,  divested  of  certain  restraining  features,  but  essentially  un- 
changed in  regard  to  energy  and  intelligence,  the  principal  change 
being  an  alteration  in  the  direction  of  interests  from  within,  outward. 
Prefrontal  lobotomy  is  radical  treatment,  not  only  figuratively  but 
literally  in  that  it  reaches  down  to  the  roots  of  the  personality.  If  the 
neurosis  or  psychosis  has  its  roots  in  an  overwhelming  consciousness 
of  the  self,  and  if  this  consciousness  is  intractable  and  disabling,  then 
prefrontal  lobotomy  may  bring  peace  to  the  warring  mind  and  a  new 
lease  on  life.  [15,  118  f.] 

Here  are  some  of  their  illustrations  of  the  alteration  in  per- 
sonality after  prefrontal  lobotomy:8 

A  timid,  restrained  hypochondriacal  woman  now  joins  in  the  party 
with  her  daughter,  is  able  to  dance  until  after  midnight  with  her 
daughter's  friends,  cook  supper  for  them,  and  send  them  home  in 
the  midst  of  jests  and  wise  cracks.  She  is  no  longer  conscious  of 
the  fatigue  nor  is  she  very  much  aware  that  there  is  such  a  thing  as 
dignity  in  the  presence  of  young  people.  She  boasts  that  the  boys 
come  to  the  house  on  account  of  her.  A  former  school  teacher,  hav- 
ing suffered  from  involutional  depression  for  six  years,  sits  uncon- 
cernedly among  a  group  of  physicians  while  we  are  describing  to 
them  her  suicidal  attempts,  chronic  alcoholism,  her  erratic  behavior, 
and  hare-brained  plans  for  an  artists'  colony,  her  alcoholic  father,  and 
her  psychotic  mother  and  sister,  and  then  smilingly  states  to  the  doc- 
tors: "I  have  to  drink  alone  because  I  get  perfectly  nasty  in  public." 
[15,  116] 

Bennett,  Keegan,  and  Wilbur  have  reported  successful  use  of 
the  operation  in  four  cases  of  "aggressive  paranoid  schizophrenia." 
[6]  And  Hebb  and  Penfield  have  written  of  a  patient  who  de- 
veloped epileptic  seizures  after  being  struck  on  the  forehead,  who 

s  Other  detailed  accounts  of  the  effect  of  the  operation  on  social  behavior  will 
be  found  in  [16,  ch.  13]. 


434  BREAKDOWNS  OF  THE  EGO 

was  "irresponsible,  childishly  stubborn,  restless  and  forgetful"  [24, 
422],  and  who  showed  considerable  restoration  of  his  normal  per- 
sonality and  intellectual  capacity  after  a  partial  lobectomy  which 
removed  approximately  one  third  of  each  frontal  lobe.  The  dif- 
ferent effects  of  surgical  insult  reported  by  Brickner  as  contrasted 
to  those  reported  by  Freeman  and  Watts,  Hebb  and  Penfield,  and 
other  variations  found  in  the  literature  are  generally  agreed  to  be 
due  to  the  particular  nature  of  the  operation  performed.  In  spite 
of  the  physiological  mysteries  that  still  baffle  the  expert  concerned 
with  the  mechanisms  of  the  brain,  there  is  no  disagreement  on  the 
fact  that  even  partial  removal  of  the  frontal  lobes  can  and  does 
significantly  alter  an  individual's  personality,  significantly  change 
the  behavior  and  thought  that  makes  him  his  normal  "self." 

In  this  chapter  we  have  seen  that  the  genetically  formed  ego, 
which  is  re-formed  during  adolescence  and  which  is  for  the  adult 
so  largely  composed  of  a  constellation  of  personal  and  social  values, 
can  and  does  disintegrate,  dissolve  or  break  down,  under  certain 
conditions.  Definite  signs  of  ego-deterioration  or  breakdown  are 
observable.  These  vary  according  to  the  particular  situation  to 
which  a  person  is  exposed  and,  within  limits,  are  subject  to  in- 
dividual differences  of  temperament,  ability,  and  the  like.  The 
few  examples  of  ego-breakdown  reviewed  here  give  further  evi- 
dence that  the  ego  is  not  an  innate  entity,  that  it  is  not  fixed  and 
static,  and  that  its  change  and  dissolution,  like  its  formation  and 
re-formation,  result  from  the  relationship  between  the  individual 
and  his  social  environment.  And  the  cases  of  ego-breakdown  re- 
sulting from  definite  organic  disturbances  or  physiological  imbal- 
ance are  further  evidence  of  the  fact  that  ego-striving  is  rooted 
in  the  human  organism,  is  made  possible  because  of  its  poten- 
tiality for  functioning  on  a  conceptual  level,  and  is  therefore 
neither  a  primary  instinct  in  the  sense  of  hunger  or  sex  nor  the 
manifestation  of  some  mystic  force. 

REFERENCES 

1.  ALLPORT,  G.  W.,  J.  S.  BRUNER,  and  E.  M.  JANDORF,  Personality  under  social 
catastrophe:  ninety  life-histories  of  the  Nazi  revolution,  Character  and  Per- 
sonality, 1941,  10,  1-22. 


435 

2.  ANGELL,  R.  C.,  The  family  Encounters  the  Depression,  New  York:  Scribner's, 

copyright  1936. 

3.  ANGYAL,  A.,  The  experience  of  the  body-self  in  schizophrenia,  Arch.  Neurology 

and  Psychiatry,  1936,  35,  1029-53. 

4.  BALES,   R.   F.,   Social   therapy  for  a  social  disorder — compulsive   drinking, 

/.  Social  Issues,  1945,  1,  14-22. 

5.  BEISWANGER,  G.,  Martha  Graham:  a  perspective,  in  Martha  Graham,  by  Bar- 

bara Morgan,  Duell,  Sloan  and  Pearcc,  copyright  1941. 

6.  BENNETT,  A.  E.,  J.  J.  KEEGAN,  and  C.  B.  WILBUR,  Prefrontal  lobotomy  in 

chronic  schizophrenia,  J.  Am.  Med.  Assoc.,  1943,  123,  809-13. 

7.  BETTELHEIM,  B.,  Individual  and  mass  behavior  in  extreme  situations,  /.  Ab- 

norm,  and  Soc.  Psychol,  1943,  38,  417-52. 

8.  BRICKNER,  R.  M.,  The  Intellectual  Functions  of  the  Frontal  Lobes,  New  York: 

Macmillan,  1936,  quotations  by  permission  of  the  author. 

9.  COBB,  S.,  Personality  as  affected  by  lesions  of  the  brain,  in  Personality  and  the 

Behavior  Disorders  (J.  McV.  Hunt,  ed.),  vol.  1,  New  York:  Ronald,  copy- 
right 1944,  ch.  18. 

10.  COUSINS,  S.,  To  Beg  1  Am  Ashamed,  New  York:  Vanguard,  copyright  1938. 

11.  DUBLIN,  L.  I.,  and  B.  BUNZEL,  To  Be  or  Not  To  Be,  New  York:  Smith  Sc 

Haas,  1933. 

12.  EISENBERG,  P.,  and  P.  F.  LAZARSFELD,  The  psychological  effects  of  unemploy- 

ment, Psychol.  Bull,  1938,  35,  358-90. 

13.  FARIS,  R.  E.  L.,  Reflections  of  social  disorganization  in  the  behavior  of  a 

schizophrenic  patient,  Am.  J.  SocioL,  1944,  50,  134-41. 

14.  FRANZ,  S.  L,  Persons  One  and  Three,  New  York:  McGraw-Hill,  copyright 

1933. 

15.  FREEMAN,  W.,  and  J.  W.  WATTS,  The  frontal  lobes  and  consciousness  of  the 

self,  Psychosomatic  Med.,  1941,  3,  111-19. 
16. Psychosurgery,  Baltimore:  Thomas,  1942. 

17.  GELB,  A.,  and  K.   GOLDSTEIN,  Psychologische   Analysen  hirnpathologischer 

Falle:  X.  Uber  Farbennamenamnesie,  Psychol.  Forsch.,  1925,  6,  127-86. 
(Zbl  ges.  Neurol.  Psychiat.,  1925,  60,  577.) 

18.  GOLDSTEIN,  K.,  The  Organism,  New  York:  Am.  Book  Co.,  copyright  1939. 

19.  GOLDSTEIN,   K.,   and   A.   GELB,   Psychologische   Analysen   hirnpathologischer 

Falle:  I.  Zur  psychologic  des  optischen  Wahrnehmungs-  und  Erkennungs- 
vorganges,  Z.  ges.  Neurol.  Psychiat.,  1918,  41,  1-142.  (Zbl.  ges.  Neurol. 
Psychiat.,  1919,  18,  105-08.) 

20.  GOLDSTEIN,  K.,  and  M.  SCHEERER,  Abstract  and  concrete  behavior:  an  experi- 

mental study  with  special  tests,  Psychol.  Monogr.,  1941,  53,  no.  2. 

21.  GRINKER,  R.  R.,  and  J.  P.  SPIEGEL,  War  Neuroses,  Philadelphia:  Blakiston, 

copyright  1945. 
22. Men  Under  Stress,  Philadelphia:  Blakiston,  copyright  1945. 

23.  HALSTEAD,  W.  C.,  Experimental  analysis  of  the  effects  of  pre-frontal  lobectomy 

in  man,  Psychol.  Bull.,  1938,  35,  687. 

24.  HEBB,  D.  O.,  and  W.  PENFIELD,  Human  behavior  after  extensive  bilateral 

removal  from  the  frontal  lobes,  Arch.  Neurol.  Psychiat.,  1940,  44,  421-38. 

25.  HUNT,  J.  McV.,  and  C.  N.  COFER,  Psychological  deficit,  in  Personality  and  the 

Behavior  Disorders  (J.  McV.  Hunt,  ed.),  vol.  2,  New  York:  Ronald,  copy- 
right 1944,  ch.  32. 


436  BREAKDOWNS  OF  THE  EGO 

26.  ISRAELI,  N.,  Distress  in  the  outlook  of  Lancashire  and  Scottish  unemployed, 

/.  Appl.  Psychol.,  1935,  19,  67-9. 

27.  KISKER,  G.  W.,  and  G.  W.  KNOX,  The  psychopathology  of  the  ego  system, 

/.  Nerv.  and  Ment.  Diseases,  1943,  96,  66-71. 

28.  KLEBANOFF,  S.  G.,  Psychological  changes  in  organic  brain  lesions  and  ablations, 

Psychol.  Bull,  1945,  42,  585-623.. 

29.  KOFFKA,  K.,  Principles  of  Gestalt  Psychology,  New  York:  Harcourt,  Brace, 

copyright  1935. 

30.  KRAINES,  S.  H.,  The  Therapy  of  the  Neuroses  and  Psychoses,  Philadelphia: 

Lea  and  Febiger,  copyright  1943,  2d  ed. 

31.  MAULDIN,  B.,  Up  Front,  New  York:  Henry  Holt,  copyright  1945. 

32.  McDoucALL,  W.,  Outline  of  Abnormal  Psychology,  New  York:  Scribner's, 

copyright  1926. 

33.  MOWRER,  E.  R.,  Disorganization:  Personal  and  Social,  New  York:  Lippincott, 

1942. 

34.  MUNCIE,  W.,  Psychobiology  and  Psychiatry,  St.  Louis:  Mosby,  copyright  1939. 

35.  New  Yor%  Times,  September  29,  1945. 

36.  RIBOT,  T.,  Diseases  of  Memory,  New  York:  Appleton,  copyright  1881. 

37.  Les  Maladies  de  la  personnalite ,  Paris:  Alcan,  1895. 

38.  RYLANDER,  G.,  Personality  Changes  after  Operations  on  the  Frontal  Lobes:  A 

Clinical  Study  of  Thirty-two  Cases,  Copenhagen:  Ejnar  Munksgard,  1939. 

39.  SCHILDER,  P.,  Goals  and  Desires  of  Man,  New  York:  Columbia  Univ.  Press, 

copyright  1942. 

40.  SHOCK,  N.  W.,  Physiological  factors  in  behavior,  in  Personality  and  the  Be* 

havior  Disorders  (J.  McV.  Hunt,  ed,),  vol.  1,  New  York:  Ronald,  copy- 
right 1944,  ch.  19. 

41.  STOUFFER,  S.  A.,  and  P.  F.  LAZARSFELD,  Research  Memorandum  on  the  Family 

in  the  Depression,  New  York:  Social  Science  Research  Council,  Bull.  29, 
1937. 

42.  VAN  DE  VELDE,  TH.  H.,  Ideal  Marriage,  New  York:  Covici  Friede,  copyright 

1931,  quotations  by  permission  of  Random  House. 

43.  WEIGL,  E.,  Zur  psychologic  sogenannter  Abstraktionsprozesse:  Untersuchungen 

iiber  das  "Ordnen,"  Z.  Psychol.,  1927,  103,  2-45;  trans,  by  M.  J.  Rioch, 
/.  Abnorm.  &  Soc.  Psychol.,  1941,  36,  3-33. 

44.  WHITE,  W.  A.,  Outlines  of  Psychiatry,  New  York:  J.  Nervous  and  Mental  Dis- 

ease Publishing  Co.,  1913. 

45.  WILLIAMS,  R.  D.,  H.  L.  MASON,  R.  M.  WILDER,  and  B.  F.  SMITH,  Induced  thia- 

mine  (Bj)  deficiency  and  the  thiamine  requirement  of  man:  Further  observa- 
tions, Arch,  intern.  Med.,  1942,  69,  721. 

46.  ZAWADSKI,  B.,  and  P.  LAZARSFELD,  The  psychological  consequences  of  unem- 

ployment, /.  Soc.  Psychol.,  1935,  6, 224-51. 


CHAPTER  13 
SOME  ILLUSTRATIONS  FROM  LITERATURE 

It  has  often  been  pointed  out  that  novelists  and  playwrights  seem 
to  be  better  observers  of  "human  nature"  than  professional  psy- 
chologists. And  there  is  little  doubt  that  in  the  vast  storehouse 
of  literature  the  psychologist  would  find  nearly  all  of  his  observa- 
tions anticipated.  But  the  very  nature  of  the  psychologist's  job, 
unlike  that  of  the  novelist,  dramatist,  or  poet,  imposes  on  him  the 
obligation  of  going  beyond  his  observations,  accounting  for  them 
with  consistent  scientific  concepts.  His  intellectual  burden  is 
lightened  only  insofar  as  he  can  verify  his  observations,  see  what 
general  psychological  principles  can  be  drawn  from  them. 

In  literary  works  produced  in  different  countries  at  different 
periods  of  time,  there  is  abundant  illustration  that  novelists  and 
playwrights  were  fully  aware  of  the  fact  that  many  a  person's 
interests  and  desires  are  ego-involved.  Through  the  characters  or 
situations  they  build  up,  they  reveal  in  dramatic  proportions  prop- 
erties of  the  ego  that  are  similar  in  their  characteristics  to  those 
we  have  already  found  in  controlled  investigations.  And  so,  on 
the  basis  of  these  controlled  studies  and  the  knowledge  we  have 
gained  of  the  development  and  components  of  the  ego,  we  are  in 
a  position  to  handle  systematically  the  psychology  of  ego-involve- 
ments as  these  appear  in  various  literary  works.  We  must,  of 
course,  always  recognize  the  subtle  shadings  and  nuances  given  by 
any  author  to  the  feelings,  passions,  and  situations  he  portrays; 
we  must  bear  in  mind  that  the  literary  artist  is  by  and  large  deal- 
ing with  particular  and  unique  qualities  of  personal  experience. 
But  through  the  variety  of  intricate  and  distinct  episodes  and 
characterizations,  we  can  find  underlying  psychological  principles 
reflected. 

In  this  chapter  we  must  limit  ourselves  to  a  few  of  thousands 
of  examples  that  might  be  given  in  a  more  exhaustive  analysis. 

437 


438  SOME  ILLUSTRATIONS  FROM  LITERATURE 

A  thick  book  could  be  written  on  the  use  which  Shakespeare's 
genius  alone  made  of  the  psychology  of  ego-involvements.  We 
shall  proceed  here,  as  we  have  before,  from  relatively  simple  to 
relatively  complex  situations. 

We  saw  in  an  earlier  chapter  how  the  boundaries  of  the  child's 
ego  gradually  expanded  to  include  his  body,  his  room,  his  clothes, 
his  toys,  his  house,  and  so  on.  Most  of  us,  whether  children  or 
adults,  feel  that  there  is  some  little  space  we  can  call  ours,  that  is 
part  of  our  own  psychological  world,  included  in  our  own  ego- 
boundaries.  The  space  may  be  some  corner  of  a  room  where  we 
have  a  favorite  chair,  it  may  be  some  glade  in  the  woods  to  which 
we  make  recurrent  visits,  it  may  be  a  barrel  in  the  woodshed  we 
like  to  sit  on  in  the  evening  after  dinner.  Whatever  it  is,  we  come 
to  feel  that  that  space  is  not  only  ours  but  is  a  part  of  us.  If  it  is 
pre-empted  by  someone  else,  destroyed,  or  intruded  upon,  we  are 
annoyed,  we  feel  that  our  privacy,  our  selves  have  been  violated, 
injured,  or  insulted. 

This  ego-involvement  a  person  has  with  a  space  which  through 
usage  and  association  has  become  his,  is  illustrated  in  John  Stein- 
beck's Of  Mice  and  Men.  [14]  Crooks  is  a  Negro  stablehand  who 
had  his  own  little  bunk  in  the  harness  room;  Lennie,  a  big, 
kindly,  almost  moronic  individual  who  has  a  temporary  job  on 
the  place,  looked  after  by  a  faithful  friend,  but  generally  isolated 
from  the  rest  of  the  men. 

Crooks  sat  on  his  bunk.  His  shirt  was  out  of  his  jeans  in  back.  In 
one  hand  he  held  a  bottle  of  liniment,  and  with  the  other  he  rubbed 
his  spine.  Now  and  then  he  poured  a  few  drops  of  the  liniment  into 
his  pink-palmed  hand  and  reached  up  under  his  shirt  to  rub  again. 
He  flexed  his  muscles  against  his  back  and  shivered. 

Noiselessly  Lennie  appeared  in  the  open  doorway  and  stood  there 
looking  in,  his  big  shoulders  nearly  filling  the  opening.  For  a  mo- 
ment Crooks  did  not  see  him,  but  on  raising  his  eyes  he  stiffened  and 
a  scowl  came  on  his  face.  His  hand  came  out  from  under  his  shirt. 

Lennie  smiled  helplessly  in  an  attempt  to  make  friends. 

Crooks  said  sharply,  "You  got  no  right  to  come  in  my  room.  This 
here's  my  room.  Nobody  got  any  right  in  here  but  me!' 

Lennie  gulped  and  his  smile  grew  more  fawning.  "I  ain't  doing 
nothing,"  he  said.  "Just  come  to  look  at  my  puppy.  And  I  seen  your 
light,'1  he  explained. 


SOME  ILLUSTRATIONS  FROM  LITERATURE  439 

"Well,  I  got  a  right  to  have  a  light.  You  go  on  get  outta  my  room. 
I  ain't  wanted  in  the  bunk  house,  and  you  ain't  wanted  in  my  room" 
[14, 


The  favorite  theme  in  the  literature  of  all  ages  and  all  peoples 
is,  of  course,  the  love  of  a  man  and  a  woman.  Many  authors  have 
shown  that  all-embracing  passionate  complete  love  means  essen- 
tially that  a  lover  has  merged  his  or  her  ego  with  the  person  loved, 
that  the  self  of  one  person  includes  the  self  of  the  other,  that  what 
is  one's  is  also  the  other's,  that  the  separation  of  the  lovers  means 
a  separation  of  the  self  of  each.  Shakespeare,  at  the  very  end  of 
his  Measure  for  Measure,  has  the  duke  say  to  his  Isabel: 

What's  mine  is  yours,  and  what  is  yours  is  mine.  [Act  V,  Scene  1] 

And  in  Romeo  and  Juliet,  Juliet  tells  Romeo  that  the  more  she 
gives  to  him  the  more  she  has: 

Juliet.    What  satisfaction  canst  thou  have  to-night? 

Romeo.   The  exchange  of  thy  love's  faithful  vow  for  mine. 

Juliet.  I  gave  thee  mine  before  thou  didst  request  it;  And  yet  I 
would  it  were  to  give  again. 

Romeo.   Wouldst  thou  withdraw  it?  for  what  purpose,  love? 

Juliet,  But  to  be  frank,  and  give  it  thee  again.  And  yet  I  wish  but 
for  the  thing  I  have:  My  bounty  is  as  boundless  as  the  sea, 

My  love  as  deep;  the  more  I  give  to  thee,  the  more  I  have,  for  both 
are  infinite.  [Act  II,  Scene  2] 

In  Emily  Bronte's  Wuthering  Heights  [1],  Catherine  reveals  her 
complete  identity  with  Heathcliflf,  the  man  she  loves: 

.  .  .  he's  more  myself  than  1  am.  Whatever  our  souls  are  made  of, 
his  and  mine  are  the  same  .  .  .  My  great  miseries  in  this  world  have 
been  Heathcliff's  miseries,  and  I  watched  and  felt  each  from  the  be- 
ginning: my  great  thought  in  living  is  himself.  'If  all  else  perished, 
and  he  remained,  I  should  still  continue  to  be;  and  if  all  else  re- 
mained, and  he  were  annihilated,  the  universe  would  turn  to  a  mighty 
stranger:  I  should  not  seem  a  part  of  it.  My  love  for  Linton  is  like 
the  foliage  in  the  woods:  time  will  change  it,  I'm  well  aware,  as  win- 
ter changes  the  trees.  My  love  for  Heathcliflf  resembles  the  eternal 
rocks  beneath:  a  source  of  litde  visible  delight,  but  necessary.  Nelly, 

1  All  italics  in  literary  passages  quoted  arc  ours.  Words  in  bold  type  italicized 
in  originals. 


440  SOME  ILLUSTRATIONS  FROM  LITERATURE 

I  am  Heathdiftl  He's  always,  always  in  my  mind:  not  as  a  pleasure, 
any  more  than  I  am  always  a  pleasure  to  myself,  but  as  my  own 
being.  [1,  68  f.] 

Eugene  O'Neill  in  The  Great  God  Brown  [10]  at  the  end  of  the 
play  shows  the  undying  love  of  a  wife  for  her  dead  husband.  In 
this  scene,  Margaret,  the  wife,  on  a  moonlight  night  nostalgically 
revisits  the  scene  where  her  husband,  Dion,  proposed  to  her.  She 
asks  her  boys  to  leave  her,  and  in  the  last  scene  of  the  play  she  says: 

So  long  ago!  And  yet  I'm  still  the  same  Margaret.  It's  only  our 
lives  that  grow  old.  We  are  where  centuries  only  count  as  seconds  and 
after  a  thousand  lives  our  eyes  begin  to  open — (she  looks  around  her 
with  a  rapt  smile) — and  the  moon  rests  in  the  sea!  I  want  to  feel  the 
moon  at  peace  in  the  sea!  I  want  Dion  to  leave  the  sky  for  me!  I 
want  him  to  sleep  in  the  tides  of  my  heart!  (She  slowly  takes  from 
under  her  cloak,  from  her  bosom,  as  if  from  her  heart,  the  mask  o£ 
Dion  as  it  was  at  the  last  and  holds  it  before  her  face)  My  lover!  My 
husband!  My  boy!  You  can  never  die  till  my  heart  dies!  You  will 
live  forever!  You  are  sleeping  under  my  heart!  I  feel  you  stirring  in 
your  sleep,  forever  under  my  heart.  (She  kisses  him  on  the  lips  with  a 
timeless  kiss,)  [10,  98] 

The  feeling  of  passionate  lovers  has  been  caught  by  Ernest  Hem- 
ingway in  his  moving  story  of  the  Spanish  Civil  War,  For  Whom 
the  Bell  Tolls  [4].  Robert  Jordan,  an  American  fighting  with  the 
Spanish  Republican  forces,  and  the  Spanish  girl,  Maria,  whom  he 
affectionately  calls  "Guapa"  and  "Rabbit,"  are  lying  together  at 
night  in  the  open  woods. 

Then  they  were  together  so  that  as  the  hand  on  the  watch  moved, 
unseen  now,  they  %new  that  nothing  could  ever  happen  to  the  one 
that  did  not  happen  to  the  other,  that  no  other  thing  could  happen 
more  than  this;  that  this  was  all  and  always;  this  was  what  had  been 
and  now  and  whatever  was  to  come.  This,  that  they  were  not  to 
have,  they  were  having.  They  were  having  now  and  before  and 
always  and  now  and  now  and  now.  Oh,  now,  now,  now,  the  only 
now,  and  above  all  now,  and  there  is  no  other  now  but  thou  now 
and  now  is  thy  prophet.  Now  and  forever  now.  Come  now,  now, 
for  there  is  no  now  but  now.  Yes,  now.  Now,  please  now,  only 
now,  not  anything  else  only  this  now,  and  where  are  you  and  where 
am  I  and  where  is  the  other  one,  and  not  why,  not  ever  why,  only 


SOME  ILLUSTRATIONS  FROM  LITERATURE  441 

this  now;  and  on  and  always  please  then  always  now,  always  now, 
for  now  always  one  now;  one  only  one,  there  is  no  other  one  but  one 
now,  one,  going  now,  rising  now,  sailing  now,  leaving  now,  wheeling 
now,  soaring  now,  away  now,  all  the  way  now,  all  of  all  the  way 
now;  one  and  one  is  one,  is  one,  is  one,  is  one,  is  still  one,  is  still  one, 
is  one  descendingly,  is  one  softly,  is  one  longingly,  is  one  fyndly,  is 
one  happily,  is  one  in  goodness,  is  one  to  cherish,  is  one  now  on  earth 
with  elbows  against  the  cut  and  slept-on  branches  of  the  pine  tree 
with  the  smell  of  the  pine  boughs  and  the  night;  to  earth  conclusively 
now,  and  with  the  morning  of  the  day  to  come.  Then  he  said,  for 
the  other  was  only  in  his  head  and  he  had  said  nothing,  "Oh,  Maria, 
I  love  thee  and  I  thank  thee  for  this."  [4,  379] 

And  at  the  end  of  the  book  when  Jordan  takes  his  final  leave 
of  Maria,  this  identity  of  the  two  lovers  as  one  is  again  brought 
out. 

"Guapa,"  he  said  to  Maria  and  took  hold  of  her  two  hands.  .  .  . 

"Listen.  We  will  not  go  to  Madrid  now  but  /  go  always  with  thee 
wherever  thou  goest.  Understand  ?" 

She  said  nothing  and  pushed  her  head  against  his  cheek  with  her 
arms  around  him. 

"Listen  to  this  well,  rabbit,"  he  said.  He  knew  there  was  a  great 
hurry  and  he  was  sweating  very  much,  but  this  had  to  be  said  and 
understood.  "Thou  wilt  go  now,  rabbit.  But  I  go  with  thee.  As 
long  as  there  is  one  of  us  there  is  both  of  us.  Do  you  understand?" 

"Nay,  I  stay  with  thee." 

"Nay,  rabbit.  What  I  do  now  I  do  alone.  I  could  not  do  it  well 
with  thee.  //  thou  goest  then  I  go,  too.  Do  you  not  see  how  it  is? 
Whichever  one  there  is,  is  both!' 

"I  will  stay  with  thee." 

"Nay,  rabbit.  Listen.  That  people  cannot  do  together.  Each  one 
must  do  it  alone.  But  if  thou  goest  then  I  go  with  thee.  It  is  in  that 
way  that  I  go  too.  Thou  wilt  go  now,  I  know.  For  thou  art  good 
and  kind.  Thou  wilt  go  now  for  us  both!' 

"But  it  is  easier  if  I  stay  with  thee,"  she  said.    "It  is  better  for  me." 

"Yes.  Therefore  go  for  a  favor.  Do  it  for  me  since  it  is  what  thou 
canst  do." 

"But  you  don't  understand,  Roberto.  What  about  me?  It  is  worse 
for  me  to  go." 

"Surely,"  he  said.   "It  is  harder  for  thee.   But  I  am  thee  also  now!9 

She  said  nothing. 


442  SOME  ILLUSTRATIONS  FROM  LITERATURE 

He  looked  at  her  and  he  was  sweating  heavily  and  he  spoke  now, 
trying  harder  to  do  something  than  he  had  ever  tried  in  all  his  life. 

"Now  you  will  go  for  us  both,"  he  said.  "You  must  not  be  selfish, 
rabbit.  You  must  do  your  duty  now." 

She  shook  her  head. 

"You  are  me  now,"  he  said.    "Surely  thou  must  feel  it,  rabbit." 

"Rabbit,  listen,"  he  said.  "Truly  thus  I  go  too.  I  swear  it  to  thee." 
[4,  463] 

In  the  libretto  written  for  Richard  Strauss's  opera  Der  Rosen- 
\avdier  by  Hugo  von  Hofmannsthal  [15],  Octavian  in  talking  to 
the  Marschallin  at  the  beginning  of  the  first  act,  describes  the 
fusion  which  occurs  between  the  "I"  and  the  "You"  of  lovers.2 

.  .  ,  You,  you — what  means  this  "you"?  or  "you  and  I"? 

What  sense  has  this? 

These  are  words,  mere  words,  are  they  not?    It  is  for  you  to  say! 

But  still,  there  is  something  in  them; 

A  dizziness,  a  pulling,  a  longing  and  urging, 

A  languishing  and  burning; 

The  way  my  hand  approaches  yours, 

The  will  toward  you,  the  embracing  of  you, 

I  am  what  "wills"  toward  you; 

But  the  "I"  is  lost  in  the  "you"  .  .  . 

I  am  your  boy,  but  when  I  lose  sight  and  hearing — 

Where  is  your  boy?  [15,  2] 

In  a  familiar  passage  from  the  balcony  scene  of  Romeo  and 
Juliet,  Shakespeare  shows  how  ego-involved  a  person's  name  can 
become,  how  his  name  stands  for  his  identification  and  status,  how 
difficult  it  is  to  separate  the  person  from  what  his  name  stands  for, 
and  how  the  fate  of  two  people  who  loved  each  other  was  affected 
by  the  established  value  judgments  of  families  that  were  bitter 
enemies.  Romeo's  ego  is  one  constellation  as  a  member  of  his 
family,  a  different  constellation  as  a  lover.  [Act  II,  Scene  2] 

Juliet*    O  Romeo,  Romeo!  wherefore  art  thou  Romeo? 
Deny  thy  father,  and  refuse  thy  name; 
Or,  if  thou  wilt  not,  be  but  sworn  my  love, 
And  I'll  no  longer  be  a  Capulet. 

2  This  passage  was  called  to  our  attention  by  Oliver  Strunk  who  also  made  the 
English  translation  for  us. 


SOME  ILLUSTRATIONS  FROM  LITERATURE  443 

Romeo  (aside).   Shall  I  hear  more,  or  shall  I  speak  at  this? 

Juliet  'Tis  but  thy  name  that  is  my  enemy; 

Thou  art  thyself  though,  not  a  Montague. 

What's  Montague?  it  is  nor  hand,  nor  foot, 

Nor  arm,  nor  face,  nor  any  other  part 

Belonging  to  a  man.   O!  be  some  other  name: 

What's  in  a  name?  that  which  we  call  a  rose 

By  any  other  name  would  smell  as  sweet; 

So  Romeo  would,  were  he  not  Romeo  call'd, 

Retain  that  dear  perfection  which  he  owes 

Without  that  title.   Romeo t  doff  thy  name; 

And  for  that  name,  which  is  no  part  of  thee, 

TaJ(e  all  myself. 

In  a  story  of  Frankie,  a  12-year-old  girl  just  entering  adolescence, 
Carson  McCullers  gives  a  penetrating  account  of  the  acute  feeling 
of  isolation  Frankie  experienced  as  a  result  of  the  situation  she 
lived  through  during  a  summer.  Frankie  was  too  young  to  be 
in  girls'  clubs;  too  old  for  John  Henry,  the  six-year-old  cousin 
she  had  to  be  with;  confused  and  angered  by  Berenice,  the  Negro 
maid  who  talked  about  her  four  husbands  and  made  fun  of 
Frankie.  When  her  older  brother  returned  from  Alaska,  married 
a  girl  from  Winter  Hill,  a  city  100  miles  from  the  home  town, 
Frankie  identified  herself  romantically  and  completely  with  the 
brother  and  his  bride.  McCullers  shows  us  how  Frankie's  for- 
merly lonely  world  suddenly  expanded  to  include  the  bride  and 
groom,  how  for  the  first  time  in  her  life  she  felt  she  could  now 
regard  herself  as  part  of  a  "we/*  and  how  the  newly  extended  self 
seemed  to  dissociate  when  this  "we"  she  had  become  went  to  stay 
in  Winter  Hill.  [7] 

For  a  long  time  now  her  brother  and  the  bride  had  been  at  Winter 
Hill  They  had  left  the  town  a  hundred  miles  behind  them,  and  now 
were  in  a  city  far  away.  They  were  them  and  in  Winter  Hill,  to- 
gether, while  she  was  her  and  in  the  same  old  town  all  by  herself.  The 
long  hundred  miles  did  not  make  her  sadder  and  make  her  feel  more 
far  away  than  the  knowing  that  they  were  them  and  both  together 
and  she  was  only  her  and  parted  from  them,  by  herself.  And  as  she 
sickened  with  this  feeling  a  thought  and  explanation  suddenly  came  to 
her,  so  that  she  knew  and  almost  said  aloud:  They  are  the  we  of  me. 
Yesterday,  and  all  the  twelve  years  of  her  life,  she  had  only  been 


444  SOME  ILLUSTRATIONS  FROM  LITERATURE 

Frankic.  She  was  an  I  person  who  had  to  walk  around  and  do  things 
by  herself.  All  other  people  had  a  we  to  claim,  all  other  except  her. 
When  Berenice  said  we,  she  meant  Honey  and  Big  Mama,  her  lodge, 
or  her  church.  The  we  of  her  father  was  the  store.  All  members  of 
clubs  have  a  we  to  belong  to  and  talk  about.  The  soldiers  in  the  army 
can  say  we,  and  even  the  criminals  on  chain-gangs.  But  the  old 
Frankie  had  had  no  we  to  claim,  unless  it  would  be  the  terrible  sum- 
mer we  of  her  and  John  Henry  and  Berenice — and  that  was  the  last 
we  in  the  world  she  wanted.  Now  all  this  was  suddenly  over  with 
and  changed.  There  was  her  brother  and  the  bride,  and  it  was  as 
though  when  first  she  saw  them  something  she  had  known  inside  of 
her:  They  are  the  we  of  me.  And  that  was  why  it  made  her  feel  so 
queer,  for  them  to  be  away  in  Winter  Hill  while  she  was  left  all  by 
herself;  the  hull  of  the  old  Frankie  left  there  in  the  town  alone. 
[7,  50] 

The  importance  of  ego-involvement  with  the  same  values  if  a 
friendship  or  love  relationship  are  to  be  lasting  can  be  illustrated 
in  Sinclair  Lewis'  Cass  Timberlane.  [5]  The  following  passage  is 
part  of  a  conversation  between  Cass  Timberlane  and  his  wife, 
Jinny,  just  after  Jinny  has  admitted  to  Cass  that  she  had  become 
Bradd  Criley's  mistress.  She  has  told  Cass  she  is  going  to  leave 
him  and  go  back  to  Bradd  whom  she  intends  to  marry.  Here 
Lewis  points  out  how  the  allegiance  of  one  person  to  a  set  of 
norms  another  person  does  not  respect  makes  for  incompatibility; 
how  behavior  (in  this  case  Bradd's  truthfulness)  can  be  judged 
only  within  a  referential  frame. 

[Jinny]  "Does  charm  seem  to  you  such  a  bad  quality  for  a  girl  to 
have  in  a  husband  ?" 

— I'm  not  coming  off  so  well  in  the  argument.  And  this  is  a  life- 
death  struggle  to  hold  her,  not  just  a  squabble.  [Thinks  Cass  to  him- 
self] 

"Yes,  I  think  it  is  bad,  when  it's  deliberately  turned  on  and  off,  as  it 
is  with  a  blackguard  like  Criley." 

"You  mean  'heel,'  don't  you?  You  know,  when  you  say  he's  a  heel, 
you're  talking  like  a  man  and  it  doesn't  mean  a  thing  to  a  woman, 
unless  she's  half -man  herself.  Very  few  women  care  a  hang  about  the 
laws  or  the  social  rules.  What  they  love  in  a  man  is  the  feeling  that 
he  isn't  merely  with  them,  but  that  he  is  them,  and  feels  and  thinks  as 
they  do  before  they've  finished  thinking  it.  What  people  like  you 


SOME  ILLUSTRATIONS  PROM  LITERATURE  44$ 

detest  about  the  heels,  the  outlaws,  is  that  they  don't  give  a  hoot  for  the 
idiotic  rules  that  you've  set  up  to  protect  your  own  awkwardness,  which 
comes  from  your  never  really  being  completely  one  with  a  woman,  but 
always  remaining  a  little  aside  from  her,  noticing  how  good  you  a^re  or 
how  bad.  And  expecting  her  to  do  what— Bradd  just  laughs  when 
I'm  unpunctual,  and  maybe  you  can't  trust  what  he  says,  but  with  me 
he's  always  truthful!"  [5,  335] 

In  the  same  book  is  exemplified  the  substitute  ego-gratification 
one  person  can  have  through  another  person's  success.  In  this 
instance,  one  woman  puts  herself  in  the  place  of  another  woman 
who  has  won  the  affections  of  the  man  she  herself  loves.  The 
following  conversation  takes  place  at  a  dinner  party  given  for  Cass 
Timberlane  and  Jinny.  Cass  asks  his  hostess  to  seat  him  beside 
Chris  Grau,  a  woman  who  loves  him  and  whom  everyone  in  town 
had  expected  him  to  marry. 

"Cass!  Are  you  a  competent  husband  for  any  girl  as  fine  and  win- 
ning as  Jinny?" 

"I  don't  know.   I  hope  so." 

"You've  got  to  be!  For  my  sake,  too.  Cass,  she's  my  understudy. 
No,  she's  me;  she  represents  me,  she  is  me,  in  the  only  love-affair  I'll 
ever  have.  Are  you  gentle  enough  for  her  and  tolerant  enough  and 
imaginative  enough  and  flexible  enough?"  .  .  . 

"Cass,  you  do  love  her,  don't  you!  I'm  glad.  Do  love  her.  If  you 
ever  for  one  minute  wanted  to  love  me  or  anything  in  me,  then  love 
me  now  in  her"  [5,  179 /.] 

A  more  complicated  variation  of  this  same  theme  is  found  in 
The  Great  God  Brown  where  Dion,  shortly  before  his  death, 
taunts  Brown  for  his  love  of  Margaret,  Dion's  wife,  and  her  chil- 
dren, and  for  buying  off  Dion's  mistress  because  they  all  love  him, 
Dion.  Brown  has  just  admitted  his  love  for  Margaret.  Dion  an- 
tagonizes Brown  by  suggesting  that  Brown  can  have  the  love  of 
Margaret  and  the  children  only  if  Brown  can  make  Margaret  and 
the  children  feel  that  Dion  still  lives  through  him. 

Dion  (with  a  terrible  composure).  No!  That  is  merely  the  appear- 
ance, not  the  truth!  Brown  loves  me!  He  loves  me  because  I  have 
always  possessed  the  power  he  needed  for  love,  because  I  am  love!  .  .  . 

Dion  (sinking  in  his  chair,  more  and  more  weakly).  I'm  done.  My 
heart,  not  Brown—  (Mockingly)  My  last  will  and  testament!  Heave 


446  SOME  ILLUSTRATIONS  FROM  LITERATURE 

Dion  Anthony  to  William  Brown — for  him  to  love  and  obey — for  him 
to  become  me — then  my  Margaret  will  love  me — my  children  will  love 
me—Mr,  and  Mrs.  Brown  and  sons,  happily  ever  after!  (Staggering 
to  his  full  height  and  looking  upward  defiantly)  Nothing  more — but 
Man's  last  gesture — by  which  he  conquers — to  laugh!  Ha —  (He 
begins,  stops  as  if  paralyzed,  and  drops  on  his  knees  by  Brown's  chair, 
his  mask  falling  off,  his  Christian  Martyr's  face  at  the  point  of  death) 
Forgive  me,  Billy.  Bury  me,  hide  me,  forget  me  for  your  own  happi- 
ness! May  Margaret  love  you!  May  you  design  the  Temple  of  Man's 
soul!  Blessed  are  the  meek  and  the  poor  in  spirit!  (He  kisses  Brown's 
feet— then  more  and  more  weakly  and  childishly)  What  was  the 
prayer,  Billy?  I'm  getting  so  sleepy  .  .  .  [10,  64 /.] 

In  our  formulations,  the  devotion  one  person  has  for  another  is 
due  to  the  fact  that  this  other  person  is  included  within  one's  own 
ego  boundaries,  has  become  a  personal  value  constituting  part  of 
one's  own  ego.  Thus  if  our  friend  suffers,  we  suffer  with  him; 
if  he  is  successful  and  achieves  a  fame  he  desires,  we  are  happy 
and  proud.  Great  friendships  recorded  in  history  or  literature  or 
seen  in  everyday  life,  involve  a  mutual  devotion,  reveal  an  over- 
lapping or  inclusion  of  one  person's  ego  with  the  other's.  In  his 
tragedy  Hippolytus  [3],  Euripides  portrays  the  devotion  of  a  nurse 
for  her  mistress.  The  mistress,  Phaedra,  has  fallen  passionately 
in  love  with  Hippolytus,  a  proud  individual,  symbol  of  chastity 
and  asceticism.  Phaedra,  a  married  woman,  has  vowed  to  die 
rather  than  let  her  passion  for  Hippolytus  overcome  her  and  dis- 
grace her.  The  nurse  is  ignorant  of  the  cause  of  Phaedra's  deter- 
mination to  die.  Phaedra  sees  that  her  own  death  will  bring  death 
to  her  devoted  nurse. 

[Nurse]  There  then  I  cover  thee;  but  when  will  death  hide  my  body 
in  the  grave?  Many  a  lesson  length  of  days  is  teaching  me.  Yea, 
mortal  men  should  pledge  themselves  to  moderate  friendships  only, 
not  to  such  as  reach  the  very  heart's  core;  affection's  ties  should  be 
light  upon  them  to  let  them  slip  or  draw  them  tight.  For  one  poor 
heart  to  grieve  for  twain t  as  I  do  for  my  mistress,  is  a  burden  sore  to 
bear.  Men  say  that  too  engrossing  pursuits  in  life  more  oft  cause  dis- 
appointment than  pleasure,  and  too  oft  are  foes  to  health.  Wherefore 
I  do  not  praise  excess  so  much  as  moderation,  and  with  me  wise  men 
will  agree.  .  .  . 


SOME  ILLUSTRATIONS  FROM  LITERATURE  447 

(At  the  mention  of  his  [Hippolytus'j  name  Phaedra's  attention  is 
suddenly  caught.) 

Phaedra.  Oh!   Oh! 

Nurse.   Ha!  doth  that  touch  the  quick? 

Phaedra.  Thou  hast  undone  me,  nurse;  I  do  adjure  by  the  gods, 
mention  that  man  no  more. 

Nurse.  There  now!  thou  art  thyself  again,  but  e'en  yet  refusest  to 
aid  thy  children  and  preserve  thy  life. 

Phaedra.  My  babes  I  love,  but  there  is  another  storm  that  buffets 
me. 

Nurse.   Daughter,  are  thy  hands  from  bloodshed  pure? 

Phaedra.   My  hands  are  pure,  but  on  my  soul  there  rests  a  stain. 

Nurse.    The  issue  of  some  enemy's  secret  witchery  ? 

Phaedra.   A  friend  is  my  destroyer,  one  unwilling  as  myself. 

Nurse.    Hath  Theseus  wronged  thee  in  any  wise? 

Phaedra.   Never  may  I  prove  untrue  to  him! 

Nurse.  Then  what  strange  mystery  is  there  that  drives  thee  on  to 
die? 

Phaedra.    O,  let  my  sin  and  me  alone!  'tis  not  'gainst  thee  I  sin. 

Nurse.   Never  willingly!  and,  if  I  fail,  'twill  rest  at  thy  door. 

Phaedra.   How  now  ?  thou  usest  force  in  clinging  to  my  hand. 

Nurse.   Yea,  and  I  will  never  loose  my  hold  upon  thy  knees. 

Phaedra.  Alas  for  thee!  my  sorrows,  shouldst  thou  learn  them, 
would  recoil  on  thee. 

Nurse.    What  \eener  grief  for  me  than  failing  to  win  thee? 

Phaedra.    'Twill  be  death  to  thee;  though  to  me  that  brings  renown. 


In  one  of  Phaedra's  speeches,  Euripides  reveals  the  tense  and 
tragic  conflict  that  arises  when  sexual  passions  run  counter  to 
highly  cherished  values  that  have  become  a  part  of  one's  self.  In 
the  following  speech  we  find  Phaedra,  convinced  that  she  will  not 
be  able  to  master  and  subdue  her  love  for  Hippolytus,  deciding 
on  death  rather  than  a  life  in  which  she  could  no  longer  identify 
herself  with  values  that  to  her  were  paramount.  These  values 
include  her  status  and  position  as  the  member  of  a  noble  family, 
her  adherence  to  the  norms  of  marital  fidelity,  her  love  for  her 
children,  and  the  unstained  reputation  she  wants  to  leave  behind 
when  she  dies. 


448  SOME  ILLUSTRATIONS  FROM  LITERATURE 

When  love  wounded  me,  I  bethought  me  how  I  best  might  bear 
the  smart.  So  from  that  day  forth  I  began  to  hide  in  silence  what  I 
suffered.  For  I  put  no  faith  in  counsellors,  who  know  well  to  lecture 
others  for  presumption,  yet  themselves  have  countless  troubles  of  their 
own.  Next  I  did  devise  noble  endurance  of  these  wanton  thoughts, 
striving  by  continence  for  victory.  And  last  when  I  could  not  succeed 
in  mastering  love  hereby,  methought  it  best  to  die;  and  none  can 
gainsay  my  purpose.  For  fain  I  would  my  virtue  should  to  all  appear, 
my  shame  have  jew  to  witness  it.  I  knew  my  sickly  passion  now;  to 
yield  to  it  I  saw  how  infamous;  and  more,  I  learnt  to  know  so  well 
that  I  was  but  a  woman,  a  thing  the  world  detests.  Curses,  hideous 
curses  on  that  wife  who  first  did  shame  her  marriage-vow  for  lovers 
other  than  her  lord!  Twas  from  noble  families  this  curse  began  to 
spread  among  our  sex.  For  when  the  noble  countenance  disgrace, 
poor  folk  of  course  will  think  that  it  is  right.  Those  too  I  hate  who 
make  profession  of  purity,  though  in  secret  reckless  sinners.  How  can 
these,  queen  Cypris,  ocean's  child,  e'er  look  their  husbands  in  the 
face?  do  they  never  feel  one  guilty  thrill  that  their  accomplice,  night, 
or  the  chambers  of  their  house  will  find  a  voice  and  speak?  This  it 
is  that  calls  on  me  to  die,  kind  friends,  that  so  I  may  ne'er  be  found 
to  have  disgraced  my  lord,  or  the  children  I  have  borne;  no!  may  they 
grow  up  and  dwell  in  glorious  Athens,  free  to  speak  and  act,  heirs  to 
such  fair  fame  as  a  mother  can  bequeath.  For  to  know  that  father  or 
mother  has  sinned  doth  turn  the  stoutest  heart  to  slavishness.  This 
alone,  men  say,  can  stand  the  buffets  of  life's  battle,  a  just  and  virtu- 
ous soul  in  whomsoever  found.  For  time  unmasks  the  villain  soon  or 
late,  holding  up  to  them  a  mirror  as  to  some  blooming  maid.  'Mongst 
such  may  I  be  never  seen!  [3,  774] 

Both  laboratory  experiments  and  other  controlled  investigations 
have  shown  us  how  the  particular  constellation  of  social  values 
that  forms  so  large  a  part  of  our  egos  gives  us  our  particular  status, 
our  anchoring  and  our  identity  in  the  network  of  social  relation- 
ships around  us.  Our  standing  in  a  group  or  community,  our 
reputation,  generally  stems  from  judgments  made  by  others  of  our 
status.  If  we  are  relatively  satisfied  or  proud  of  the  status  or  an- 
choring we  have  achieved,  then  any  reflection  or  disparagement 
of  our  status  disturbs  our  ego,  upsets  us,  makes  us  feel  insecure, 
angry,  deflated.  Shakespeare  shows  this  in  the  well-known  pas- 
sage, spoken  by  lago  in  Othello: 


SOME  ILLUSTRATIONS  FROM  LITERATURE  449 

lago.  Good  name  in  man  and  woman,  dear  my  lord, 

Is  the  immediate  jewel  of  their  souls: 

Who  steals  my  purse  steals  trash;  'tis  something,  nothing; 

'Twas  mine,  'tis  his,  and  has  been  slave  to  thousands; 

But  he  that  filches  from  me  my  good  name 

Robs  me  of  that  which  not  enriches  him, 

And  maf(es  me  poor  indeed.  [Act  III,  Scene  3] 

In  those  primitive  and  more  advanced  societies  where  class  or 
caste  lines  exist,  we  find  that  in  nearly  all  established  communities 
one  of  the  determinants  of  a  person's  status  is  the  family  he  be- 
longs to.  The  particular  place  one's  own  family  has  in  the  hier- 
archy of  community  values  can  and  very  frequently  does  provide 
an  ego-anchorage.  Hence  one  of  the  quickest  and  surest  ways  for 
an  individual  in  these  societies  to  rise  in  the  social  scale  is  to  marry 
into  a  family  already  solidly  established  in  a  superior  position.  To 
guard  against  a  breakdown  of  class  and  status  lines,  it  was  the 
accepted  practice  in  many  societies  in  earlier  years  for  parents  to 
choose  the  husbands  or  wives  for  their  children.  Although  this 
custom  has  generally  broken  down  in  advanced  countries,  there 
are  still  subtle  and  indirect  methods  parents  can  and  do  use  to 
minimize  the  danger  that  a  child  will  fall  in  love  with  someone 
"beneath"  him  and  to  maximize  the  opportunities  for  social  inter- 
course between  children  of  the  same  class:  A  family  will  live  in 
the  "right"  neighborhood,  children  will  be  sent  to  the  "right" 
school  or  summer  camp,  and  so  on.  But  when,  in  spite  of  these 
precautions,  a  child  from  a  "superior"  class  breaks  out  of  bounds 
and  decides  to  marry  someone  beneath  him,  considerable  conster- 
nation is  likely  to  arise.  Parents  feel  that  their  status  will  be  low- 
ered by  any  such  action,  they  suffer  an  ego-disturbance,  an  ego- 
failure. 

The  difficulties  and  conflicts  arising  either  from  the  attempt  of 
a  person  to  marry  into  a  class  he  regards  as  "higher,"  or  the  threat 
to  the  ego  of  a  person  in  a  "superior"  class  because  a  child  is  going 
to  marry  "beneath"  him,  has  been  a  recurrent  theme  in  literature, 
especially  in  American  novels  in  the  past  few  decades.  We  will 
cite  here  only  two  of  these  more  recent  examples. 

The  first  is  from  Marquand's  The  Late  George  Apley.  [6]  The 
Apleys  could  count  themselves  among  the  uppercrust  of  Boston 


450  SOME  ILLUSTRATIONS  FROM  LITERATURE 

society— they  lived  on  Beacon  Hill,  sent  their  sons  to  Harvard, 
and  so  on.  For  generations  this  had  been  the  case,  and  George 
Apley  had  sufficient  independent  means  to  maintain  his  position 
well.  His  son  John  had  secretly  married  the  recently  divorced 
Louise  McCullogh.  The  father  was  shocked  when  he  heard  the 
news  for,  as  he  said,  he  had  always  believed  that  "J°hn  was 
basically  sound."  He  thought  Louise  came  from  a  "reasonably 
good"  New  York  family.  On  learning  of  John's  marriage,  George 
Apley  wrote  to  a  close  friend: 

"I  had  hoped  better  things  of  John  but  now  that  he  has  in  quite  a 
real  sense  left  us,  I  feel  more  than  ever  a  sense  of  responsibility  toward 
the  family."  [6,  307] 

Later  on  George  Apley  learned  more  about  Louise's  family,  and 
his  whole  estimate  of  her  changed.  The  following  paragraph  is 
taken  from  a  letter  he  wrote  to  John  after  acquiring  the  added 
information: 

"/  cannot  for  the  life  of  me  see  why  you  did  not  tell  me  in  the  first 
place  that  Louise,  whom  I  am  growing  more  and  more  anxious  daily 
to  set  eyes  upon,  was  one  of  the  Hogarths  of  Connecticut.  This,  of 
course,  maizes  a  very  great  difference,  foolish,  perhaps,  in  your  eyes, 
but  not  in  mine.  It  has  always  been  a  most  important  thing  with 
me  to  place  a  person.  Many  of  us  are  accused  unjustly  of  being  snob- 
bish. This  is  not  the  case.  The  so-called  snobbishness  is  generally 
due  to  one's  inability  to  place  the  subject  of  it  in  the  accepted  social 
scale.  Louise's  being  a  Hogarth  places  her  perfectly.  I  am  sure  that 
the  man  McCullogh  must  have  been  a  very  bad  hat."  [6,  308] 

Christopher  Morley,  in  his  Kitty  Foyle  [9],  outlines  in  bold 
relief  the  thoughts  and  feelings  of  an  independent  self-sufficient 
girl,  Kitty  Foyle,  the  daughter  of  a  former  cricket  club  coach  and 
night  foreman  at  a  machine  shop,  who  falls  in  love  with  Wynne- 
wood  Strafford  VI,  son  of  an  old  Main  Line  Philadelphia  family. 
Although  Wyn  also  loves  Kitty,  she  refused  to  marry  him  because 
of  her  class  consciousness  and  class  pride,  and  he  married  a  Phila- 
delphia socialite  in  the  approved  fashion.  The  following  excerpts 
are  from  a  few  of  the  scenes  Kitty  recalls  some  years  after  Wyn's 
marriage. 


SOME  ILLUSTRATIONS  FROM  LITERATURE  451 

Wyn  wanted  to  take  me  out  to  Darby  Mill.  He  had  planned  it  all 
beforehand,  they  were  throwing  a  house-party  out  there  and  I'd  had 
a  note  from  his  mother.  Naturally  I  had  some  sense,  I  could  even 
see  in  the  way  her  handwriting  went  up  and  down  it  was  a  fever 
chart,  but  Wyn  wouldn't  take  No. 

It  was  a  mistake.  Of  course  Wyn  had  done  what  any  man  would, 
told  everybody  to  be  lovely  to  me  and  they  were  so  god  damn  lovely 
I  could  have  torn  their  eyes  out.  /  was  the  only  one  that  wasn't  in 
the  union.  That  crowd,  if  they  stopped  to  think  about  it,  would 
reckon  that  Ben  Franklin  was  still  a  boy  from  the  wrong  side  of  the 
tracks,  so  what  would  they  think  about  me.  Somebody  wanted  to 
know  if  I  was  one  of  the  Iglehart  Foyles  from  Baltimore  or  the 
Saltonstall  Foyles  from  Pride's  Crossing.  I  said  no  pride  ever  crossed 
our  family,  except  when  the  old  man  carried  his  bat  against  Merion 
C.  C.  .  .  .  /  \new  either  I  or  the  rest  of  them  didn't  belong,  and  the 
embarrassment  went  around  the  dinner  table  all  wrapped  up  in  a 
napkin  like  that  wine  bottle  the  butlers  carried.  [9,  787  /.] 

Later  in  the  book,  Kitty  recalls  another  episode. 

"Well  then  I've  got  to  tell  you,"  Wyn  said.  "Uncle  Kennett  has  a 
big  idea,  he  wanted  to  explain  it  to  you  himself.  He  says  you're  just 
exactly  the  girl  for  me,  Kitty,  and  the  girl  the  family  needs,  and  he 
wants  to  send  you  back  to  college  for  a  year  and  then  maybe  go  abroad 
a  year  and  meanwhile  I'll  try  to  get  some  education  myself  and  be 
ready  for  you." 

Oh  Jesusgod  I  don't  know  exactly  how  you  said  it,  Wyn.  It  was 
something  like  that.  My  poor  baby,  how  could  you  know  what  that 
would  do  to  me  the  way  I  was  just  then.  Maybe  that  nice  old  man 
with  his  thee  talk  could  have  sold  it  to  me;  I  don't  know.  I  had  a  kind 
of  picture  of  some  damned  family  conference  and  the  Straffords  and 
their  advisers  trying  to  figure  out  how  the  curse  was  going  to  be  taken 
off  Kitty  Foyle.  So  that  was  it,  they  were  going  to  buy  the  girl  with  an 
education,  and  polish  off  her  rough  Frankford  edges,  were  they,  and 
make  her  good  enough  to  live  with  stuffed  animals'  heads  and  get 
advertised  in  the  Ledger.  I  can  still  see  your  face,  my  poor  baby,  when 
I  turned  on  you.  I  felt  hot  inside  my  throat  and  on  the  rims  of  my 
ears. 

"You  can  tell  Uncle  Ken  he's  a  white  slaver.  Listen,  Wyn  Strafford, 
I'll  be  your  girl  whenever  I  feel  like  it  because  I  love  you  from  hell  to 
breakfast.  But  I  wouldn't  join  the  little  tin  family  if  every  old  Quaker 
with  an  adding  machine  begged  me  to.  No,  not  if  they  all  went  back 


452  SOME  ILLUSTRATIONS  FROM  LITERATURE 

to  college  and  got  themselves  an  education.  So  they  tried  to  sell  you 
the  idea  they'd  trim  up  Kitty  so  she  could  go  to  the  Assembly  and 
tna\e  Old  Philadelphia  Family  out  of  her,  hey?  Cut  her  out  of  a  copy 
of  Vogue  and  give  her  a  charge  account  and  makf  a  Main  Line  doll 
out  of  her.  They  can't  do  that  to  Kitty  Foyle.  Jesusgod,  that's  what 
they  are  themselves,  a  bunch  of  paper  dolls." 

...  By  God,  I'll  improve  you  all  I  want  but  you  can't  improve  me. 
[9,2*lff.] 

The  way  in  which  a  person's  status  or  role  in  society,  rather  than 
any  intrinsic  characteristics  or  qualities  of  his  personality,  deter- 
mines how  others  react  to  him,  and,  in  turn,  how  he  is  deeply 
influenced  by  them  in  his  ego-inflations  has  often  provided  the 
basis  for  comedy,  tragedy,  and  for  literature  that  aims  to  convey 
a  "moral."  G.  H.  Mead  has  pointed  out  that  we  frequently  judge 
people  by  projecting  ourselves  into  the  status  role  we  attribute 
to  the  person  being  judged  [8].  George  Bernard  Shaw  seems  to 
take  particular  pleasure  in  showing  up  hypocrisy  by  bringing  into 
his  plays  pointed  references  to  this  effect  of  status  on  our  evalua- 
tion of  an  individual.  In  The  Devil's  Disciple  [11],  Richard 
Dudgeon,  who  has  been  regarded  by  his  mother  and  the  rest  of 
the  Puritan  community  as  a  renegade  and  dubbed  "The  Devil's 
Disciple,"  is  found  unexpectedly  to  have  inherited  his  father's 
property.  When  Richard  obeys  the  summons  of  the  Presbyterian 
divine,  Anderson,  and  visits  the  latter's  house,  he  finds  to  his  sur- 
prise that  he  is  treated  quite  civilly  for  only  recently  Anderson 
had  roundly  condemned  him  for  his  sins.  Just  before  the  follow- 
ing incident,  Richard  is  apologizing  to  the  minister  and  his  wife 
for  the  wetness  of  his  coat,  due  to  his  walk  in  the  rain: 

Anderson.  Take  it  off,  sir;  and  let  it  hang  before  the  fire  a  while: 
my  wife  will  excuse  your  shirtsleeves.  Judith:  put  in  another  spoonful 
of  tea  for  Mr.  Dudgeon. 

Richard  (eyeing  him  cynically).  The  magic  of  property  t  Pastor! 
Are  even  you  civil  to  me  now  that  I  have  succeeded  to  my  fathers 
estate? 

Judith  throws  down  the  spoon  indignantly.  [11,  Act  II] 

And  not  only  is  our  judgment  of  a  person  affected  by  the  status 
he  holds,  but  his  own  judgment  of  himself,  what  he  regards  him- 
self to  be,  can  be  and  often  is  determined  by  the  reactions  of 


SOME  ILLUSTRATIONS  FROM  LITERATURE  453 

Others  to  him.  If  a  person  is  treated  as  though  he  were  a  person 
with  high  status,  then  he  is  likely  to  regard  himself  as  such  (at 
least  for  the  time  being) ;  if  he  is  treated  as  though  he  had  a  low 
status  in  the  particular  hierarchy  of  values  used  as  the  referential 
framework,  then  he  is  disappointed,  insulted,  and  so  on.  Shaw 
calls  attention  to  this  form  of  ego-involvement  in  his  Pygmalion 
[12].  A  poor  London  flower  girl,  Liza,  is  made  into  a  lady  by  the 
speech  expert,  Higgins,  who  teaches  her  in  his  Wimpole  Street 
studio  to  speak  and  act  like  a  lady.  Higgins  treats  Liza  as  an  ex- 
perimental subject  and  he  is,  as  Shaw  describes  him,  "careless 
about  himself  and  other  people,  including  their  feelings."  Par- 
ticipating in  the  experiment  is  a  visiting  speech  expert,  Pickering, 
a  sensitive  man  with  the  qualities  of  a  gentleman.  Just  before 
the  following  dialogue  takes  place,  Liza  is  telling  Pickering,  in 
front  of  Higgins,  that  although  Higgins  did  teach  her  to  speak 
as  a  lady  would  "it  was  just  like  learning  to  dance  in  the  fashion- 
able way:  there  was  nothing  more  than  that  in  it."  Liza  con- 
tinues with  the  question: 

But  do  you  know  what  began  my  real  education? 

Pickering.   What? 

Liza.  Your  calling  me  Miss  Doolittle  that  day  when  I  first  came  to 
Wimpole  Street.  That  was  the  beginning  of  self-respect  for  me.  And 
there  were  a  hundred  little  things  you  never  noticed,  because  they 
came  naturally  to  you.  Things  about  standing  up  and  taking  off  your 
hat  and  opening  doors—- 
Pickering. Oh,  that  was  nothing. 

Liza.  Yes:  things  that  showed  you  thought  and  felt  about  me  as  if 
I  were  something  better  than  a  scullery-maid;  though  of  course  I 
know  you  would  have  been  just  the  same  to  a  scullery-maid  if  she  had 
been  let  into  the  drawing  room.  You  never  took  off  your  boots  in 
the  dining  room  when  I  was  there. 

Pickering.  You  mustn't  mind  that.  Higgins  takes  off  his  boots  all 
over  the  place. 

Liza.  I  know.  I  am  not  blaming  him.  It  is  his  way,  isn't  it?  But 
it  made  such  a  difference  to  me  that  you  didn't  do  it.  You  sec,  really 
and  truly,  apart  from  the  things  anyone  can  pick  up  (the  dressing  and 
the  proper  way  of  speaking,  and  so  on),  the  difference  between  a  lady 
and  a  flower-girl  is  not  how  she  behaves,  but  how  she's  treated.  I  shall 


454  SOME  ILLUSTRATIONS  FROM  LITERATURE 

always  be  a  flower  girl  to  Professor  Higgins,  because  he  always  treats 
me  as  a  flower  girl,  and  always  will;  but  I  know  I  can  be  a  lady  to  you, 
because  you  always  treat  me  as  a  lady,  and  always  will.  [12,  Act  V] 

A  frequent  theme  of  tragedies  is  the  portrayal  of  the  fate  of 
individuals  who,  obeying  the  edicts  of  their  conscience  or  what 
they  regard  as  a  higher  law,  run  up  against  man-made  laws  or 
established  norms  of  their  community.  The  individual  in  these 
situations  is  shown  to  have  so  identified  himself  with  a  particular 
value  that  compromise  is  impossible  and  death  is  preferable  to  a 
life  in  which  a  radical  realteration  of  the  ego  structure  would  be 
forced.  This  is  the  situation  in  Sophocles'  play  Antigone  [13]. 
Creon  had  assumed  the  throne  of  Thebes  and  had  issued  a  procla- 
mation that  the  corpse  of  one  of  Antigone's  brothers  who  had  led 
an  armed  force  against  the  city  and  who  had  been  killed  in  com- 
bat should  remain  unburied  on  the  battlefield.  Because  of  the 
emphasis  placed  by  the  Greeks  upon  the  necessity  of  proper  funeral 
rites,  this  action  of  Creon's  was  regarded  with  particular  terror; 
it  ran  completely  counter  to  the  divine  laws  accepted  by  the  people 
at  that  time.  Antigone,  deliberately  disobeyed  Creon's  proclama- 
tion by  performing  the  necessary  burial  rites  for  her  brother's  body, 
out  of  respect  for  her  brother  and  for  a  higher  law  emanating  from 
the  gods  she  believed  in.  For  this  action,  Creon  condemned  her 
to  be  buried  alive  in  a  vaulted  grave.  The  following  excerpt  is 
from  a  speech  of  Antigone  after  she  learned  Creon's  judgment: 

Tomb,  bridal-chamber,  eternal  prison  in  the  caverned  rock,  whither 
I  go  to  find  mine  own,  those  many  who  have  perished,  and  whom 
Persephone  hath  received  among  the  dead!  Last  of  all  shall  I  pass 
thither,  and  far  most  miserably  of  all,  before  the  term  of  my  life  is 
spent.  But  I  cherish  good  hope  that  my  coming  will  be  welcome  to 
my  father,  and  pleasant  to  thee,  my  mother,  and  welcome,  brother,  to 
thee;  for,  when  ye  died,  with  mine  own  hands  I  washed  and  dressed 
you,  and  poured  drink-offerings  at  your  graves;  and  now,  Polyneices, 
'tis  for  tending  thy  corpse  that  I  win  such  recompense  as  this. 

And  yet  I  honoured  thee,  as  the  wise  will  deem,  rightly.  .  .  . 

And  what  law  of  heaven  have  I  transgressed?  Why,  hapless  one, 
should  I  look  to  the  gods  any  more,— what  ally  should  I  invoke,-— 
when  by  piety  I  have  earned  the  name  of  impious  ?*  Nay,  then,  if 


SOME  ILLUSTRATIONS  FROM  LITERATURE  455 

these  things  are  pleasing  to  the  gods,  when  I  have  suffered  my  doom, 
I  shall  come  to  know  my  sin;  but  if  the  sin  is  with  my  judges,  I  could 
wish  them  no  fuller  measure  of  evil  than  they,  on  their  part,  mete 
wrongfully  to  me.  [13,  447  f.] 

To  give  up  life  for  what  is  regarded  as  a  "higher0  life,  is,  of 
course,  found  in  the  precepts  of  various  religious  leaders.  The 
individual  who  completely  accepts  the  principles  of  these  religious 
prophets,  is  promised  a  "new"  life.  In  the  Gospel  of  St.  Matthew, 
Christ's  teaching  is  reported: 

Then  said  Jesus  unto  his  disciples,  If  any  man  will  come  after  me,  let 
him  deny  himself,  and  take  up  his  cross,  and  follow  me. 

For  whosoever  will  save  his  life  shall  lose  it:  and  whosoever  will  lose 
his  life  for  my  sake  shall  find  it. 

For  what  is  a  man  profited,  if  he  shall  gain  the  whole  world,  and 
lose  his  own  soul?  or  what  shall  a  man  give  in  exchange  for  his  soul? 
[Matt.  16:24-26] 

And  the  disciple,  Paul,  in  his  epistle  to  the  Galatians,  tells  of  the 
new  life  he  has  after  his  conversion. 

For  I  through  the  law  am  dead  to  the  law,  that  I  might  live  unto 
God. 

/  am  crucified  with  Christ:  nevertheless  I  live;  yet  not  I,  but  Christ 
liveth  in  me;  and  the  life  which  I  now  live  in  the  flesh  I  live  by  the 
faith  of  the  Son  of  God,  who  loved  me,  and  gave  himself  for  me. 
[Gal  2:19-20] 

The  transformation  of  personality  under  condition  of  extreme 
stress,  the  deprivation  of  instinctual  needs,  or  organic  changes  fur- 
nish the  story  of  many  famous  literary  characters.  Samuel  Butler 
in  The  Way  of  All  Flesh  [2]  observed  in  simple  language  one  of 
the  psychological  conditions  for  the  breakdown  of  the  ego. 

All  our  lives  long,  every  day  and  every  hour,  we  are  engaged  in 
the  process  of  accommodating  our  changed  and  unchanged  selves  to 
changed  and  unchanged  surroundings;  living,  in  fact,  in  nothing  else 
than  this  process  of  accommodation;  when  we  fail  in  it  a  little  we 
are  stupid,  when  we  fail  flagrantly  we  are  mad,  when  we  suspend  it 
temporarily  we  sleep,  when  we  give  up  the  attempt  altogether  we  die. 
In  quiet,  uneventful  lives  the  changes  internal  and  external  are  so 
small  that  there  is  little  or  no  strain  in  the  process  of  fusion  and 


456  SOME  ILLUSTRATIONS  FROM  LtTERATORE 

accommodation;  in  other  lives  there  is  great  strain,  but  there  is  also 
great  fusing  and  accommodating  power;  in  others  great  strain  with 
little  accommodating  power.  [2,  343] 

In  Hamlet,  Shakespeare  portrays  Ophelia's  madness  in  part  by 
having  her  appear  before  the  King  and  Queen  and  behaving  in 
quite  unseemly  fashion.  Clearly  her  standards  of  respect  for  these 
high  personages  had  broken  down  as  had  her  standards  of  decent 
conduct.  After  Ophelia  is  ushered  into  the  presence  of  the  King 
and  Queen,  the  following  scene  occurs: 

Ophelia.  Where  is  the  beauteous  majesty  of  Denmark? 

Queen.  How  now,  Ophelia! 

Ophelia.  How  should  I  your  true  love  know 

From  another  one? 
By  his  cockle  hat  and  staff, 

And  his  sandal  shoon. 

Queen.  Alas  I  sweet  lady,  what  imports  this  song? 
Ophelia.  Say  you  ?  nay,  pray  you,  mark. 
He  is  dead  and  gone,  lady, 

He  is  dead  and  gone; 
At  his  head  a  grass-green  turf; 

At  his  heels  a  stone. 
0,ho! 

Queen.  Nay,  but,  Ophelia,— 
Ophelia.  Pray  you,  mark. 

White  his  shroud  as  the  mountain  snow, — 

(Enter  King) 

Queen.  Alas!  look  here,  my  lord. 

Ophelia.  Larded  with  sweet  flowers; 

Which  bewept  to  the  grave  did  go 
With  true-love  showers. 

King.  How  do  you,  pretty  lady? 

Ophelia.  Well,  God 'ild  you!  They  say  the  owl  was  a  baker's  daugh- 
ter. Lord!  we  know  what  we  are,  but  know  not  what  we  may  be. 
God  be  at  your  table  I 

King.   Conceit  upon  her  father. 

Ophelia.  Pray  you,  let's  have  no  words  of  this;  but  when  they  ask 
you  what  it  means,  say  you  this: 


SOME  ILLUSTRATIONS  FROM  LITERATURE  457 

Tomorrow  is  Saint  Valentine's  day, 

All  in  the  morning  betime, 
And  I  a  maid  at  your  window, 

To  be  your  Valentine: 
Then  up  he  rose,  and  donn'd  his  clothes, 

And  dupp'd  the  chamber-door; 
Let  in  the  maid,  that  out  a  maid 

Never  departed  more. 
King.  Pretty  Ophelia! 

Ophelia.  Indeed,  la!  without  an  oath,  111  make  an  end  on  't: 
By  Gis  and  by  Saint  Charity, 
Alack,  and  fie  for  shame! 
Young  men  will  do  't,  if  they  come  to  't; 

By  Cock  they  are  to  blame. 
Quoth  she,  before  you  tumbled  me, 

You  promised  me  to  wed: 
So  would  I  ha'  done,  by  yonder  son, 

An  thou  hadst  not  come  to  my  bed. 
King.  How  long  hath  she  been  thus? 

Ophelia.  I  hope  all  will  be  well.  We  must  be  patient:  but  I  cannot 
choose  but  weep,  to  think  they  should  lay  him  i'  the  cold  ground.  My 
brother  shall  know  of  it:  and  so  I  thank  you  for  your  good  counsel. 
Come,  my  coach!  Good-night,  ladies;  good-night,  sweet  ladies;  good- 
night, good-night.  [Act  IV,  Scene  5] 

Shakespeare's  Hamlet,  one  of  the  most  complicated  and  complex 
characters  in  all  literature,  is  many-sided.  And  Shakespeare  has 
Hamlet  himself  aware  of  the  different  constellations  and  patterns 
of  values  which  constitute  the  changing  pattern  of  his  ego,  a  fact 
which  further  adds  to  the  complication.  Depending  upon  his 
mood  and  the  situation  in  which  he  finds  himself,  Hamlet  is  a 
different  person  at  different  times.  Right  after  his  famous  so- 
liloquy, "To  be  or  not  to  be,"  the  following  dialogue  takes  place 
between  Hamlet  and  Ophelia: 

Ophelia.  My  lord,  I  have  remembrances  of  yours, 

That  I  have  long  longed  to  re-deliver; 

I  pray  you,  now  receive  them. 
Hamlet.   No,  not  I; 

I  never  gave  you  aught. 


458  SOME  ILLUSTRATIONS  FROM  LITERATURE 

Ophelia.   My  honour'd  lord,  you  know  right  well  you  did; 
And,  with  them,  words  of  so  sweet  breath  compos'd 
As  made  the  things  more  rich :  their  perfume  lost, 
Take  these  again;  for  to  the  noble  mind 
Rich  gifts  wax  poor  when  givers  prove  unkind. 
There,  my  lord. 

Hamlet   Ha,  ha!  are  you  honest? 
Ophelia*  My  lord! 
Hamlet.   Are  you  fair? 
Ophelia.   What  means  your  lordship  ? 

Hamlet   That  if  you  be  honest  and  fair,  your  honesty  should  admit 
no  discourse  to  your  beauty. 

Ophelia.    Could  beauty,  my  lord,  have  better  commerce  than  with 
honesty? 

Hamlet    Ay,  truly;  for  the  power  of  beauty  will  sooner  transform 
honesty  from  what  it  is  to  a  bawd  than  the  force  of  honesty  can  trans- 
late beauty  into  his  likeness :  this  was  sometime  a  paradox,  but  now  the 
time  gives  it  proof.   I  did  love  thee  once. 
Ophelia.    Indeed,  my  lord,  you  made  me  believe  so. 
Hamlet    You  should  not  have  believed  me;  for  virtue  cannot  so 
inoculate  our  old  stock  but  we  shall  relish  of  it:  /  loved  you  not.  [Act 
III,  Scene  1] 

And  again  at  the  end  of  the  play,  just  before  he  duels  with 
Laertes,  the  one  Hamlet  asks  Laertes  forgiveness  for  any  wrong 
another  Hamlet  has  done: 

Hamlet   Give  me  your  pardon,  sir;  I've  done  you  wrong; 
But  pardon  't,  as  you  are  a  gentleman. 
This  presence  knows, 

And  you  must  needs  have  heard,  how  I  am  punish'd 
With  sore  distraction.   What  I  have  done, 
That  might  your  nature,  honour  and  exception 
Roughly  awake,  I  here  proclaim  was  madness. 
Was  't  Hamlet  wrong' d  Laertes?    Never  Hamlet: 
If  Hamlet  from  himself  be  tafen  away, 
And  when  he's  not  himself  does  wrong  Laertes, 
Then  Hamlet  does  it  not;  Hamlet  denies  it. 
Who  does  it  then?    His  madness.   If 't  be  so, 
Hamlet  is  of  the  faction  that  is  wrong'd; 
His  madness  is  poor  Hamlet's  enemy. 


REFERENCES  459 

Sir,  in  this  audience, 

Let  my  disclaiming  from  a  purposed  evil 

Free  me  so  far  in  your  most  generous  thoughts, 

That  I  have  shot  mine  arrow  o'er  the  house, 

And  hurt  my  brother.  [Act  V,  Scene  2] 

These  are  only  a  few  scattered  examples  from  literature  of  the 
ways  in  which  the  novelist  and  playwright  have  pointed  to  the 
problem  of  ego-involvement  We  have  seen  how  people  have  be- 
come attached  to  certain  values,  how  these  values  have  become  a 
part  of  them  in  the  various  situations  described.  The  allegiance 
to  conflicting  values  within  an  individual  is  the  basis  of  ego-dis- 
turbance, ego-conflicts,  just  as  the  mergence  and  overlapping  of 
values  within  the  single  individual  or  between  individuals  is  the 
basis  for  ego-satisfaction,  ego-expansion.  Detailed  and  systematic 
analyses  of  the  literature  of  all  peoples,  both  ancient  and  modern, 
would  surely  pose  for  the  psychologist  wide  varieties  of  ego-in- 
volving situations,  intricate  patterns  of  ego  constellations,  stark 
and  subtle  modifications  of  behavior  due  to  ego-involved  attitudes. 
The  self-appointed  job  of  the  novelist,  the  playwright,  and  the 
poet  is  to  portray  life,  analyze  it  impressionistically  as  a  sensitive 
observer.  The  self-appointed  job  of  the  psychologist  is  to  explain 
systematically  and  scientifically  the  thoughts,  feelings,  and  actions 
of  men.  Since  so  much  of  life  is  filled  with  ego-involved  experi- 
ences, the  literary  artist  and  the  psychologist  are  bound  to  have 
this  common  meeting  ground. 

REFERENCES 

1.  BRONTE,  E.,  Wuthering  Heights,  New  York:  Button,  copyright  1907. 

2.  BUTLER,  S.,  The  Way  of  All  Flesh,  New  York:  Dutton,  copyright  1916. 

3.  EURIPIDES,  Hippolytus,  in  The  Complete  Gree\  Drama  (Whitney  J.  Gates  and 

Eugene  O'Neill,  Jr.,  eds.),  trans,  of  E.  P.  Coleridge,  New  York:  Random 
House,  copyright  1938. 

4.  HEMINGWAY,  E.,  For  Whom  the  Bell  Tolls,  New  York:  Scribner's,  copyright 

1940;  quotations  by  permission  of  the  publisher. 

5.  LEWIS,  S.,  Cass  Timberlane,  New  York:  Random  House,  copyright  1945;  quo- 

tations by  permission  of  the  publisher. 

6.  MARQUAND,  J.  P.,  The  Late  George  Apley,  Boston:  Little,  Brown,  copyright 

1937;  quotations  by  permission  of  the  publishers. 

7.  McCuLLBRS,  C.,  The  Member  of  the  Wedding,  Boston:  Houghton  MifHin, 

copyright  1946;  quotations  by  permission  of  the  publisher. 


460  SOME  ILLUSTRATIONS  FROM  LITERATURE 

8.  MEAD,  G.  H.,  Mind,  Self  and  Society,  Chicago:  Univ.  Press,  1934. 

9.  MORLEY,  C.,  Kitty  Foyle,  Philadelphia:  Lippincott,  copyright  1939;  quotations 

by  permission  of  the  publisher. 

10.  O'NEILL,  E.,  The  Great  God  Brown,  New  York:  Boni  &  Liveright,  copyright 

1926;  quotations  by  permission  of  Random  House. 

11.  SHAW,  G.  P.,  The  Devil's  Disciple,  in  Three  Plays  for  Puritans,  New  York: 

Brentano,  copyright  1906;  quotations  by  permission  of  the  author. 
12. Pygmalion,  New  York:  Dodd,  Mead,  copyright  1916;  quotations  by  per- 
mission of  the  author. 

13.  SOPHOCLES,  Antigone,  in  The  Complete  Gree\  Drama  (Whitney  J.  Gates  and 

Eugene  O'Neill,  Jr.,  eds.),  trans,  of  R.  C.  Jebb,  New  York:  Random  House, 
copyright  1938. 

14.  STEINBECK,  J.,  Of  Mice  and  Men,  New  York:  Modern  Library,  copyright 

1937;  quotations  by  permission  of  Viking  Press. 

15.  VON  HOFMANNSTHAL,  H.,  Libretto  for  Richard  Strauss'  opera  Der  Rosenfyva- 

Her,  Berlin:  Adolph  Fiirstncr,  copyright  1910. 


CHAPTER  14 
THE  EGO  IN  PSYCHOANALYSIS 

In  casually  patterned  societies  a  social  lag  is  created  by  the  im- 
pact of  technological  developments  on  established  institutional 
controls;  on  religious,  political,  and  economic  ideology;  and  on 
norms  of  behavior.  This  is  reflected  in  the  turmoil  that  goes  on 
within  the  individual  when  he  tries  to  make  a  satisfactory  life 
adjustment.  Personal  dilemmas,  uncertainties,  and  crises  are  in- 
variable by-products  of  periods  of  social  change.  In  recent  years, 
laymen  who  have  wanted  some  "scientific"  help  in  meeting  their 
personal  problems  have  turned  to  psychology.  The  psychology 
they  appeal  to  is  very  often  some  form  of  psychoanalysis.  It  is 
perhaps  the  only  kind  of  psychology  they  have  ever  heard  of  or, 
at  least,  the  only  variety  of  psychology  that  seems  to  hold  out  any 
promise  of  explanation  or  eventual  resolution  of  their  difficulties. 
And  many  social  scientists,  too,  quite  properly  thinking  psychology 
should  be  able  to  help  in  accounting  for  anthropological,  eco- 
nomic, or  political  phenomena,  have  turned  especially  to  psycho- 
analysis and,  like  the  layman,  have  tended  to  equate  it  with  psy- 
chology. 

Psychoanalysis  has,  therefore,  a  tremendous  vogue.  Its  appeal 
seems  to  increase  with  the  general  acceleration  of  social  change. 
This  appeal  comes  partly  from  the  fact  that  psychoanalysts  have 
had  the  courage  to  deal  with  problems  of  general  interest  and  con- 
cern to  laymen  and  to  social  scientists,  and  that  they  attempt  to 
explain  phenomena  of  everyday  life  experienced  by  the  individual 
or  observed  by  the  social  scientist.  The  appeal  is  also  due  no  doubt 
partly  to  the  fact  that  they  use  a  dramatic  and  alluring  method, 
one  that  deals  with  "hidden"  and  unconscious  motives,  with  sex 
and  with  love,  with  hate  and  with  fear.  Once  an  individual  has 

been  initiated  into  the  psychoanalytical  approach  and  terminology, 

461 


462  THE  EGO  IN  PSYCHOANALYSIS 

the  search  for  the  causes  and  cures  of  personal  and  social  problems 
takes  on  all  the  thrill  and  excitement  of  detective  story  fiction. 

If  psychoanalysis  were  concerned  only  with  individual  therapy, 
we  would  not  need  to  discuss  it  in  social  psychology.  But  psycho- 
analysis does  become  relevant  and  important  for  us  because  it 
deals  with  the  problems  of  social  psychology,  offers  explanations 
of  the  various  social  phenomena  that  are  the  concern  of  the  social 
psychologist— with  groups  and  institutions,  with  religion  and  other 
ideologies,  with  war  and  revolution. 

Here  is  the  scope  of  psychoanalysis  as  seen  by  some  of  its  ex- 
ponents. 

The  relationship  of  psychoanalysis  to  social  psychology  was 
made  explicit  by  Erich  Fromm.  In  1930  he  noted  that  psycho- 
analysis made  no  false  distinctions  between  individual  and  social 
psychology.  The  basic  method  of  social  psychology  is,  he  held, 
the  same  as  the  method  of  psychoanalysis,  an  extension  of  Freud's 
early  emphasis  on  the  importance  of  the  environment  to  the  child. 
[22]  Fromm  felt  that  psychoanalysis,  by  making  a  comprehensive 
analysis  of  the  nature  of  man  as  it  works  itself  out  in  a  social 
process,  could  be  particularly  useful  in  enriching  and  giving  a 
psychological  basis  to  the  historical  materialism  of  Marx  and  En- 
gels.  [23,  45  /.]  Others  have  made  less  cautious  statements.  Jones 
writes  that  "the  prevailing  muddle"  we  see  in  man's  social  rela- 
tions is  due  more  than  anything  else  to  ignorance  of  "those  un- 
conscious forces  on  the  nature  of  which  psychoanalysis  is  throw- 
ing so  much  light."  [35, 194]  Alexander  relies  on  psychoanalysis 
as  the  cure  for  our  present  social  problems: 

The  methods  and  principles  of  dynamic  psychology  created  an 
entirely  new  field — the  science  of  human  relationships.  ...  In  the 
face  of  the  present  wholesale  manifestation  of  irrational  forces,  all  eyes 
turn  for  an  explanation  to  psychiatry,  the  science  of  irrational  human 
behavior.  [4,22/.j 

Kardiner  tells  us  that  if  the  conclusions  he  has  reached  in  applying 
psychoanalysis  to  the  study  of  various  cultures  prove  to  be  correct 
"then  it  can  be  safely  said  that  it  has  opened  up  a  new  chapter  in 
the  understanding  of  human  society,"  [37,  252] 


FREUD'S  FORMULATIONS  463 

It  is  clear,  then,  that  psychoanalysts  have  staked  a  claim  to  the 
territory  of  social  psychology.  Because  of  this,  psychoanalysis  must 
be  watched  carefully  and  checked  for  its  scientific  validity, 

FREUD'S  FORMULATIONS* 
Freud  begins  his  group  psychology  with  the  statement: 

The  contrast  between  Individual  Psychology  and  Social  or  Group 
Psychology,  which  at  a  first  glance  may  seem  to  be  full  of  significance, 
loses  a  great  deal  of  its  sharpness  when  it  is  examined  more  closely. 
[18, 1} 

From  the  very  first  Individual  Psychology  is  at  the  same  time  Social 
Psychology  as  well.  [2] 

Whatever  else  the  social  psychologist  may  think  of  Freud's 
formulations,  he  must  acknowledge  at  once  that  the  distinction 
made  by  Freud  between  social  demands  on  the  one  hand  and  the 
instinctual  urges  of  the  individual  on  the  other  is  to  his  everlasting 
credit.  This  distinction  between  the  superstructure  of  society  and 
the  needs  of  the  individual  was  not  one  of  a  rigid  sharp  dichotomy. 
Although  Freud  did  not  carry  the  dialectics  of  his  own  methods 
to  its  logical  conclusion,  did  not  follow  through  all  its  implications, 

t-Note  on  Adler.  We  are  deliberately  centering  our  attention  here  on  Freud 
and  psychoanalysts  who  stem  from  Freudian  tradition.  Our  final  evaluation  of 
the  validity  and  adequacy  of  the  psychoanalytic  interpretation  of  social  phenomena 
will,  in  its  fundamental  respects,  also  apply  to  Adlcrian  psychology.  For,  although 
Adler  rejects  Freud's  emphasis  on  sexual  instincts  and  on  the  Oedipus  complex, 
there  is  for  Adler,  as  for  Freud,  a  universal  explanation  of  behavior:  As  the  result 
of  the  child's  feeling  of  inferiority  to  his  parents  and  to  the  world  at  large,  he 
develops  a  longing  for  superiority,  "this  fiction  of  a  goal  of  superiority  so  ridicu- 
lous from  the  viewpoint  of  reality,  has  become  the  principal  conditioning  factor 
of  our  life  as  hitherto  J^nown.  It  is  this  that  teaches  us  to  differentiate,  gives  us 
poise  and  security,  molds  and  guides  our  deeds  and  activities  and  forces  our  spirit 
to  look  ahead  and  to  perfect  itself."  [1,  8]  This  goal  of  superiority  "introduces 
into  our  life  a  hostile  and  fighting  tendency."  [8]  In  addition  to  this  goal  of 
superiority  Adler  believes  that  the  individual  has  an  "eternal,  real  and  physio- 
logically rooted  community-reeling^  It  is  from  this  community  feeling  that  we 
develop  "tenderness,  love  of  neighbor,  friendship  and  love,  the  desire  for  power 
unfolding  itself  in  a  veiled  manner  and  seeking  secretly  to  push  its  way  along 
the  path  of  group  consciousness."  [9/.]  The  ego  develops  as  a  result  of  this 
early  feeling  of  inferiority,  of  the  compensating  longing  for  superiority  and  the 
impact  of  community  feeling  in  the  child. 


464  THE  EGO  IN  PSYCHOANALYSIS 

he  recognized  the  close  interrelationship  between  social  pressure 
and  the  dynamic  forces  at  work  in  the  single  individual.  He 
pointed  out  that  the  interaction  of  social  demands  on  the  individ- 
ual was  a  process  profoundly  affecting  his  thought  and  action: 
"an  individual  in  a  group  is  subjected  through  its  influence  to  what 
is  often  a  profound  alteration  in  his  mental  activity."  [18,  33]  He 
saw  that  opposing  forces  were  at  work  within  the  unity  of  a  single 
individual. 

If  a  Psychology,  concerned  with  exploring  the  predispositions,  the 
instincts,  the  motives  and  the  aims  of  an  individual  man  down  to  his 
actions  and  his  relations  with  those  who  are  nearest  to  him,  had  com- 
pletely achieved  its  task,  and  had  cleared  up  the  whole  of  these  matters 
with  their  inter-connections,  it  would  then  suddenly  find  itself  con- 
fronted by  a  new  task  which  would  lie  before  it  unachieved.  It  would 
be  obliged  to  explain  the  surprising  fact  that  under  a  certain  condition 
this  individual  whom  it  had  come  to  understand  thought,  felt,  and 
acted  in  quite  a  different  way  from  what  would  have  been  expected. 
And  this  condition  is  his  insertion  into  a  collection  of  people  which 
has  acquired  the  characteristic  of  a  "psychological  group."  [18,  5/.] 

The  social  psychologist  is  impressed,  too,  with  Freud's  honesty, 
his  willingness  to  follow  his  observations  into  whatever  theoretical 
formulation  he  thought  they  led  him.  He  hesitated  neither  to 
break  away  from  the  scientific  and  philosophical  traditions  of  his 
day,  nor  to  disagree  with  his  own  earlier  concepts  when  he  thought 
new  evidence  called  for  revisions. 

Since  the  publication  of  Freud's  The  Ego  and  The  Id  (1923)  the 
ego  has  become  especially  important  for  psychoanalysis.  Anna 
Freud's  The  Ego  and  Mechanisms  of  Defence  published  in  1937 
further  highlighted  the  concept  so  that  modern  psychoanalytic 
literature  seems  to  be  concerned  with  the  ego  to  a  significant 
degree.  After  a  sketch  of  Freud's  own  basic  formulations  we  shall 
see  where  this  led  him  and  his  followers  in  their  attempts  to 
explain  social  phenomena. 

According  to  Freud,  we  begin  with  the  unorganized,  unrefined 
id — the  source  of  all  instinctual,  primal  impulses,  the  original  ini- 
tiators of  thought  and  activity.  It  is  not  necessary  for  us  here  to 
review  the  changes  in  Freud's  own  thinking  concerning  the  nature 


FREUD'S  FORMULATIONS  465 

and  number  of  instincts  or  to  subscribe  one  way  or  another  to  the 
often  heated  discussions  of  psychoanalysts  concerning  the  relative 
importance  of  the  sexual  instinct,  the  death  instinct,  and  the  like. 
Freud  himself  has  said  that  "there  is  obviously  a  great  opportunity 
here  for  arbitrary  choice."  [17,  66]  Our  concern  here  is  with  the 
general  formulation  that  the  infant  begins  life  with  an  undisci- 
plined id,  the  core  of  his  personality,  the  instinctual  source  of 
energy.  These  instincts  seek  immediate  gratification  so  that  the 
infant,  before  he  has  been  affected  by  social  processes,  by  social 
pressures,  is  completely  dominated  by  the  pleasure  principle.  "In 
the  psychoanalytical  theory  of  the  mind  we  take  it  for  granted  that 
the  course  of  mental  processes  is  automatically  regulated  by  'the 
pleasure  principle':  that  is  to  say,  we  believe  that  any  given  process 
originates  in  an  unpleasant  state  of  tension  and  thereupon  deter- 
mines for  itself  such  a  path  that  its  ultimate  issue  coincides  with 
the  relaxation  of  this  tension,  i.e.  with  avoidance  of  'pain*  or  with 
production  of  pleasure."  [19,  1]  The  child,  then,  is  constantly 
motivated  to  reduce  the  tension  created  by  instinctual  energy. 

The  ego  develops  when  the  child  bucks  up  against  the  outside 
world,  when  the  instinctual  impulses  of  the  id  cannot  be  immedi- 
ately gratified. 

The  ego  is  that  part  of  the  id  which  has  been  modified  by  the  direct 
influence  of  the  external  world.  [21,  29] 

The  ego  is  able  to  modify  the  id  because  the  ego  is  affected  by 
perception — both  by  the  "external  perceptions"  from  the  outer 
world  and  by  the  "internal  perceptions"  yielded  by  sensations  from 
the  id.  The  job  of  the  ego,  then,  is  that 

...  of  bringing  the  influence  of  the  external  world  to  bear  upon 
the  id  and  its  tendencies,  and  endeavors  to  substitute  the  reality- 
principle  for  the  pleasure-principle  which  reigns  supreme  in  the  id. 
In  the  ego  perception  plays  the  part  which  in  the  id  devolves  upon 
instinct.  [21,  29/.] 

The  ego  sizes  up  for  the  individual  his  relation  to  the  environment, 
it  has  the  capacity  to  suppress  immediate  pleasure  (the  reality 
principle),  to  differentiate  between  the  impulses  coming  from  the 
id  and  those  coming  from  the  outer  world,  and  to  distinguish 
between  those  impulses  of  the  id  whose  immediate  gratification 


466  THE  EGO  IN  PSYCHOANALYSIS 

will  cause  pain  and  those  that  can  be  gratified  without  fear  of 
punishment. 

Perception  for  Freud  "is  not  a  merely  passive  process;  we  believe 
rather  that  the  ego  periodically  sends  out  small  amounts  of  cathec- 
tic  energy  into  the  perceptual  system  and  by  their  means  samples 
the  external  stimuli,  and  after  every  such  groping  advance  draws 
back  again."  [20,  370]  Thought  and  judgment  are,  for  Freud, 
extensions  of  this  perceptual  process  of  "groping  forward."  The 
perceptual  function  of  the  ego  has  been  clearly  summarized  by 
Alexander: 

What  can  be  said  with  certainty  about  the  ego  is  that  it  is  a  forma- 
tion of  two  perceptive  surfaces,  one  directed  toward  the  instinctual 
life  (inner  perception),  the  second  directed  toward  external  reality 
(sense  perception) .  One  main  function  of  the  ego  is  to  confront  the 
facts  of  inner  perception  with  the  results  of  sense  perception,  i.e.,  to 
bring  subjective  demands  in  harmony  with  the  external  circumstances. 
Its  tendency  is  to  find  satisfaction  for  as  many  of  the  subjective  needs 
and  wishes  as  possible  under  existing  external  circumstances.  [3, 147  /.] 

The  end  result  is,  according  to  Freud,  that  what  we  know  as 
mind  can  be  looked  upon  "as  an  unknown  and  unconscious  id, 
upon  whose  surface  rests  the  ego,  developed  from  its  nucleus  the 
perceptual-systems."  [21,  28]  This  ego  is  constantly  defending 
itself  from  the  pain  it  fears  from  the  instincts  to  which  it  is  antag- 
onistic. [15]  Freud's  ego  includes  consciousness  and  by  controlling 
"the  approaches  to  motility,  i.e.  to  the  discharge  of  excitations  into 
the  external  world"  it  "regulates  all  its  own  constituent  processes." 
[21, 15  /.]  It  is  the  source  of  repressions.  Alexander  has  summed 
up  the  operation  of  the  ego: 

The  functioning  of  the  whole  mental  apparatus  can  be  described 
approximately  as  follows:  Instinctual  needs  and  tendencies  arising 
in  the  id  tend  to  become  conscious  because  the  conscious  ego  controls 
the  motor  innervations  on  which  the  satisfaction  of  the  needs  is  de- 
pendent. A  great  part  of  the  instinctual  demands  becomes  immedi- 
ately conscious  and  finds  its  acceptance  or  rejection  after  a  process  of 
conscious  deliberation.  This  deliberation  involves  an  estimate  of  the 
external  situation  and  a  comparison  of  the  inner  demand  in  question 
with  other  conflicting  tendencies  present  in  consciousness.  [3, 148] 


FREUD'S  FORMULATIONS  467 

The  ego,  then,  gradually  gets  more  and  more  control  over  the  id. 
And  the  particular  pattern  of  the  ego's  adjustment  of  instinctual 
forces  to  the  external  world  becomes  the  fundamental  trait  of  any 
personality.  [43] 

The  id,  however,  does  not  easily  relinquish  the  objects  which 
will  give  it  immediate  gratification.  The  repression  of  instincts, 
especially  of  erotic  trends,  is  painful.  In  order  to  make  "it  easier 
for  an  object  to  be  given  up"  [21, 36]  the  ego  substitutes  one  object 
for  another  so  the  id  can  achieve  cathexis.  Thus  the  ego  becomes 
"a  precipitate  of  abandoned  object-cathexis"  and  "contains  a  rec- 
ord of  past  object-choices."  [21,  36] 

"The  ego  is  formed  to  a  great  extent  out  of  identifications  taking 
the  place  of  cathexes  on  the  part  of  the  id  which  have  been 
abandoned."  [21,  68]  If  the  ego's  object-identifications  become 
too  numerous  and  too  incompatible  with  one  another,  intense 
pathological  conditions  result,  but  even  for  the  relatively  normal 
individual  these  "conflicts  between  the  different  identifications  into 
which  the  ego  is  split  up"  [21,  39]  are  accompanied  by  definite 
affective  properties. 

The  ego  can  gradually  get  control  over  the  id  through  this 
process  of  transforming  erotic  object-choices.  Libido  is  gradually 
withdrawn  from  the  id  as  the  object-cathexes  of  the  id  are  turned 
into  "ego  constructions."  [21,  82]  The  ego  thus  becomes  "the  true 
and  original  reservoir  of  the  libido."  [19,  66]  This  flow  of  the 
libido  into  the  ego  gives  rise  to  the  Freudian  mechanism  of  sec- 
ondary narcissism.  For  Freud,  not  only  self-love  but  love  for 
others  is  derived  from  this  libido  within  the  ego;  being  in  love 
means  "that  the  object  draws  a  part  of  the  narcissistic  ego-libido 
to  itself."  [18,725] 

The  earliest  and  the  most  important  identification  a  child  makes 
is  with  the  parents  who  are  "the  first  object-cathexes  of  the 
id."  [21,  69]  When  the  child  discerns  that  one  parent  is  an  ob- 
stacle to  the  gratification  of  the  sexual  desire  it  has  with  respect  to 
the  other  parent,  then  the  Oedipus  complex  appears.  The  ego's 
transformation  of  this  "first  and  most  important  identification  of 
all"  [21,  39]  precipitates  within  the  ego  itself  a  "special  office"  [68] 
— the  superego.  The  superego  is  the  "heir  to  the  Oedipus  com- 
plex." [21,  69]  The  cornerstone  of  the  superego  is,  as  Bychowski 


468  THE  EGO  IN  PSYCHOANALYSIS 

has  expressed  it,  "the  Images  of  persons  who  have  guided  the  child 
and  were  its  source  of  norms."  [9,  313]  Since  the  child  was 
dominated  by  its  parents  and  compelled  to  obey  them,  the  super- 
ego becomes  the  arbiter  of  what  shall  and  shall  not  be  allowed, 
the  source  of  authority  and  prohibitions.  When  the  superego 
functions  in  consciousness  it  is  known  as  conscience  or  guilt. 

This  precipitate,  the  superego,  has  for  Freud  a  special  position 
within  the  ego: 

The  broad  general  outcome  of  the  sexual  phase  governed  by  the 
Oedipus  complex  may,  therefore,  be  taken  to  be  the  forming  of  a 
precipitate  in  the  ego,  consisting  of  these  two  identifications  [father 
identification  and  mother  identification]  in  some  way  combined  to- 
gether. This  modification  of  the  ego  retains  its  special  position;  it 
stands  in  contrast  to  the  other  constituents  of  the  ego  in  the  form  of 
an  ego  ideal  or  superego.  [21,  44,  italicized  in  original] 

Freud  maintained  that  the  Oedipus  complex  is  universal.  He 
believed  that  the  id  could  be  inherited,  that  in  it  "are  stored  up 
vestiges  of  the  existences  led  by  countless  former  egos"  [21,  52], 
that  the  identification  with  the  father  (parent)  "takes  place  in  the 
prehistory  of  every  person."  [21,  39]  Although  many  analysts  do 
not  subscribe  to  Freud's  phylogenetic  theory,  the  universality  of 
the  Oedipus  complex  derived  from  early  parent-child  relationships 
is  still  a  basic  tenet  for  most  psychoanalysts  today.2  In  a  review  of 
the  history  of  the  Oedipus  complex,  for  example,  Boehm  discusses 
the  fields  of  literature,  biography,  and  the  primitive  myths  re- 
ported by  modern  ethnologists  and  concludes  that  all  these  various 
types  of  data  support  the  Oedipus  complex  as  a  phenomenon  that 
pervades  everyone's  unconscious.  [7] 

The  commanding  position  of  the  superego  in  mental  life  is,  for 
Freud,  due  to  the  fact  that  it  represents  "not  merely  a  deposit  left 
by  the  earliest  object-choices  of  the  id;  it  also  represents  an  ener- 
getic reaction-formation  against  those  choices."  [21,  44]  To  re- 
press the  Oedipus  complex  is  "no  easy  task"  for  the  individual. 
The  child  sees  the  father  "as  the  obstacle  to  realization  of  the 

2  Some  notable  and  important  exceptions  of  the  pervading  psychoanalytic  view 
concerning  the  origin  and  function  of  the  Oedipus  complex  are  considered  later 
in  this  chapter. 


FREUD'S  FORMULATIONS  469 

Oedipus  wishes."  [21,  45]  In  order  to  carry  out  this  extraordi- 
narily difficult  repression,  "the  child's  ego  brought  in  a  reinforce- 
ment" [21,  45] :  it  created  within  itself  the  character  of  the  father 
to  repress  the  Oedipus  complex.  And  the  more  tense  the  Oedipus 
complex  was  in  the  beginning  and  "the  more  rapidly  it  succumbed 
to  repression  (under  the  influence  of  discipline,  religious  teaching, 
schooling  and  reading)  the  more  exacting  later  on  is  the  domina- 
tion of  the  super-ego  over  the  ego— in  the  form  of  conscience  or 
perhaps  of  an  unconscious  sense  of  guilt."  [21,  45]  The  ego  at- 
tempts to  free  itself  from  the  dominance  of  the  superego  by  sub- 
stituting new  persons  or  new  relationships  as  representative  of  the 
parents  or  parental  relations  that  lie  behind  the  superego.  The 
result  is  that  "conflicts  and  struggles  which  had  evolved  during 
childhood  repeat  themselves  albeit  between  other  players  and  in  a 
new  shape."  [9,  313] 

In  spite  of  Freud's  characterization  of  the  superego  as  something 
with  a  "special  position"  within  the  ego,  as  something  that  "stands 
in  contrast  to  the  other  constituents  of  the  ego,"  psychoanalysts  and 
even  Freud  himself  sometimes  seem  to  have  difficulty  in  making  a 
clear  distinction  between  ego  and  superego,  in  telling  where  one 
leaves  off  and  the  other  begins.  For  example,  in  correcting  his 
earlier  view  that  the  superego  has  the  function  of  testing  the  reality 
of  things,  Freud  said: 

The  view  that  the  testing  of  reality  is  rather  one  of  the  functions 
of  the  ego  itself  would  fit  in  perfectly  with  what  we  know  of  the 
relations  of  the  ego  to  the  world  of  perception.  [21,  34,  italics  ours] 

And  what  Freud  refers  to  as  the  "double  aspects"  of  the  ego  ideal 
(superego)  inevitably  introduces  certain  confusion  in  terms:  for 
the  superego  not  only  results  from  the  repression  of  the  Oedipus 
complex  but  is  itself  the  mechanism  which  effected  the  repression. 
It  is  not  infrequent  to  find  the  ego  or  the  superego  referred  to  in 
psychoanalytic  literature  in  the  form  of  (super)  ego.  [33,  307] 
Anna  Freud  says  that  the  superego  can  be  distinguished  from  the 
ego  only  when  it  is  obvious  that  the  former  is  critical  of  the  lat- 
ter. [15]  Alexander,  referring  to  his  earlier  distinction  between 
the  unconscious  superego  and  the  conscious  ego  ideal,  says: 


470  THE  EGO  IN  PSYCHOANALYSIS 

It  seems  to  me  questionable  whether  one  should  consider  the  ego- 
ideal  more  closely  connected  with  the  super-ego,  as  its  continuation  in 
the  consciousness,  or  more  allied  to  the  actual  ego  .  .  .  the  ego-ideal 
is  not  a  completely  separate  unit,  since  it  is  hard  to  differentiate  be- 
tween conscious  values,  ideals,  guiding  principles  and  the  rest  of  the 
ego.  On  the  other  hand,  it  is  also  difficult  to  make  a  sharp  distinction 
between  the  entirely  unconscious,  almost  automatic  influences  of  the 
super-ego  and  those  more  or  less  conscious  ones  which  direct  our 
decisions  and  general  conduct.  [3,  145  f.] 

Fromm  believes  that  the  difference  between  the  ego  and  the  super- 
ego is  that  the  latter  has  an  emotional  character  derived  from  the 
incompatibility  of  instinctual  impulses  and  social  necessities.  [25, 
95]  Homey,  rejecting  Freud's  conception  of  the  origin  of  the 
Oedipus  complex,  feels  that  the  superego  should  be  regarded  as  a 
special  need  for  neurotic  individuals  rather  than  as  an  agency 
within  the  ego  itself.  The  individual's  standards  of  behavior 
which  Freud  derives  from  the  superego  are,  for  Homey,  "not  ego- 
alien  but  are  an  integral  part  of  the  self.  To  them  the  'super-ego* 
has  but  a  superficial  resemblance."  [30,  230] 

WHERE  FREUDIAN  THEORY  LEADS  IN  SOCIAL  PSYCHOLOGY 

Our  brief  sketch  of  Freud's  theory  of  ego  development  has  been 
necessary  to  give  us  a  background  for  understanding  the  applica- 
tion of  his  formulations  to  social  phenomena.  As  we  said  previ- 
ously, from  the  point  of  view  of  the  layman  and  also  of  some  social 
scientists,  one  of  the  great  attractions  of  psychoanalysis  is  its  appar- 
ent ability  to  explain  almost  any  behavior— whether  on  the  indi- 
vidual or  social  level.  We  will  cite  here  a  few  examples  of  Freudian 
interpretations  of  problems  that  concern  the  social  psychologist. 

Consider,  first  of  all,  Freud's  own  interpretations  of  the  forma- 
tion and  function  of  groups.  Individual  participation  in  a  group 
is  one  way  in  which  a  person  can  release  his  instinctual  impulses. 
All  groups  have,  therefore,  a  "libidinal  constitution."  [18,  79  f.] 
It  is  instinctual  energy  that  makes  group  formation  possible. 

A  group  is  clearly  held  together  by  a  power  of  some  kind:  and  to 
what  power  could  this  feat  be  better  ascribed  than  to  Eros  who  holds 
together  everything  in  the  world?  [18,  40] 


WHERE  FREUDIAN  THEORY  LEADS  IN  SOCIAL  PSYCHOLOGY         471 

In  group  activity  individuals  substitute  for  their  own  ego  ideals 
a  common  object  with  which  they  identify  themselves— hence  indi- 
vidual members  of  a  group  identify  themselves  with  one  another. 

Social  feeling  rests  on  the  foundation  of  identifications  with  others, 
on  the  basis  of  an  ego-ideal  in  common  with  them.  [21,  49] 

Freud  thus  accounts  for  institutional  groupings  such  as  the  army 
and  the  church.  Since  each  individual  "has  built  up  his  ego  ideal 
upon  the  most  various  models"  [18, 101],  an  individual  can  become 
part  of  a  number  of  different  groups.  For  Freud,  identification 
with  more  or  less  lasting  groups  (that  is,  racial  or  nationality)  is 
explained  by  his  belief  that  the  id  can  be  inherited  and  that  "when 
the  ego  forms  its  superego  out  of  the  id,  it  may  perhaps  only  be 
reviving  images  of  egos  that  have  passed  away."  [21,  52] 

For  Alexander  the  whole  process  of  an  individual's  adjustment 
to  his  social  environment  is  to  be  thought  of  in  terms  of  the 
incorporation  of  parental  images  into  the  personality: 

This  process  of  identification  with  the  parents  and  the  incorporation 
of  their  image  into  the  mental  apparatus  is  the  process  which  we 
usually  call  adjustment  to  the  social  environment.  [3, 143] 

For  Reik  all  social  tendencies  are  transformations  of  the  selfish  and 
aggressive  drives  of  the  ego,  so  that  the  ego  seeks  further  gratifica- 
tion in  attempting  to  overcome  group,  nationality,  and  class  differ- 
ences. [46,  755]  The  highest  fulfillment  of  this  particular  function 
of  ego-striving  is,  for  Reik,  what  we  call  love.  Love  is  the  projec- 
tion of  our  ego-ideal,  it  is  "an  escape  from  oneself"  [46,  59],  so  that 
we  really  fall  in  love  with  people  whom  we  would  like  to  be. 

Another  example  of  a  psychoanalytic  interpretation  concerned 
with  one  feature  of  social  life  is  FenichePs  carefully  worked-out 
account  of  the  function  of  money  in  our  society  and  our  desire  to 
accumulate  it.  [12]  Disagreeing  with  Ferenczi  and  Roheim  that 
money  was  originally  invented  for  the  specific  purpose  of  satisfy- 
ing an  erotogenic  (anal)  instinct,  Fenichel  regards  money  as  a 
social  institution  first  external  to  the  individual.  The  reason  the 
individual  in  our  society  reacts  to  money  as  he  does  is  as  follows. 
The  infant  considers  its  mother's  breast  and  later  its  own  faeces 
as  parts  of  its  ego.  Denial  of  the  breast  and  loss  of  faeces  therefore 
become  identified  with  injury  to  one's  own  bodily  ego. 


472  THE  EGO  IN  PSYCHOANALYSIS 

The  desire  to  possess  a  great  deal  appears  thus  to  be  a  direct  expres- 
sion of  the  narcissistic  need  to  enlarge  as  much  as  possible  the  compass 
of  one's  own  ego.  [12,  79/.] 

Our  possessions  therefore  "are  an  expanded  portion  of  the  ego." 
[80]  The  accumulation  of  possessions  is  seen  connected  primarily 
to  anal  eroticism  because  "in  the  anal  sphere  holding  back  and 
accumulating  can  afford  an  experience  of  erogenous  pleasure."  [82] 
The  same  author  accounts  for  social  class,  the  division  into  "haves" 
and  "have-nots,"  by  the  efforts  of  the  "have-nots"  to  regain  their 
lost  feeling  of  infantile  omnipotence.  This  they  can  do  by  identi- 
fying themselves  with  persons  who  represent  characterizations  of 
their  own  superego.  [13] 

A  psychoanalytic  explanation  of  war  has  been  offered  by 
Waelder  [54].  All  mass  formation,  he  believes,  is  a  process  of 
regression  where  external  principles  or  authorities  take  the  place 
of  more  mature  internalized  demands  which  the  individual's  con- 
science imposes  upon  him  as  a  substitution,  in  their  turn,  of  the 
earlier  commands  and  prohibitions  of  parents.  In  mass  formations, 
then,  men  can  act  like  children  again,  giving  vent  to  the  impulses 
of  hate  and  destruction  inherent  in  them.  Social  conflicts,  includ- 
ing wars,  are  therefore  due  to  the  "frailty  of  human  reason"  [46] 
caused  by  this  infantile  regression. 

The  psychology  of  revolution  has  been  given  special  attention 
by  Ernest  Jones.  [35]  The  characteristic  of  revolutionary  change 
is,  he  says,  destructiveness.  And  the  reason  for  this  destructiveness 
is  the  impulse  "to  displace  the  old,  or,  more  specifically,  the  Oedi- 
pus wish  to  kill  the  father."  [198]  Jones  recognizes  the  important 
role  of  revolutionary  leaders  whom  he  feels  are  people  with  no 
qualms  about  murdering  the  "father  figure."  This  is  true  of  revo- 
lutionary leaders,  because  they  possess  in  high  degree  the  para- 
noid's mechanism  of  projection  which  is  for  them  a  "defence 
against  sex  inversion,  or— more  strictly— against  the  incorporated 
'bad  objects'  resulting  from  this  inversion."  [201]  Led  on  by 
leaders  with  these  homosexual  tendencies,  participants  in  a  revolu- 
tionary movement  substitute  the  "concept  of  society"  for  their  own 
parents  whom  they  alternately  loved  and  injured.  Revolutionary 
leaders  have  the  appeal  they  do  in  critical  times,  because  it  is 


WHERE  FREUDIAN  THEORY  LEADS  IN  SOCIAL  PSYCHOLOGY         473 

during  these  critical  times  that  individuals  become  most  sensitive 
to  their  own  "deep  seated  sense  of  guilt." 

The  rise  of  Fascism  has  been  accounted  for  on  the  basis  of  the 
prolonged  infantile  sexuality  said  to  be  especially  characteristic  of 
the  working  people  and  the  Kleinbuergertum.  The  general  frus- 
trations of  this  group  led  them  to  accept  the  authority  of  a  leader 
as  a  compensating  identification.  [44]  Their  normal  "id-drives" 
were  increasingly  denied  expression  and  the  authority  of  the  super- 
ego was  diminished.  Hence  people  indulged  themselves  in  ways 
formerly  forbidden.  But  such  indulgences  only  created  new  guilt 
feelings  which  demanded  in  their  turn  self -punishment  or  sadistic 
punishment.  The  personal  satisfaction  accompanying  this  punish- 
ment became  itself  a  further  reason  for  unconscious  indulgence. 
By  creating  enemies  that  could  be  chastised  and  attacked,  there- 
fore, Naziism  was  able  to  restore  the  feeling  of  self-righteousness. 
All  the  hate  and  destructiveness  of  the  infant  was  transferred  to 
new  objects.  In  Hitler,  the  Kleinbuerger  found  an  adequate  pa- 
rental symbol  and  by  identification  with  Hitler  was  able  to  forget 
the  weakness  of  his  own  infantilism.  [51,  106-109]  Those  most 
susceptible  to  Nazi  propaganda  were  "those  who  have  neither 
securely  established  their  own  manhood  and  independence  of  the 
Father  nor  have  been  able  to  combine  the  instincts  of  sexuality  and 
love  in  their  attitude  towards  the  Mother  or  other  women."  [34,  6] 

Another  modern  application  of  psychoanalysis  is  Jones's  account 
of  the  psychology  of  Quislingism.  [34]  The  Quisling  identifies 
himself  with  the  formidable  father  image  which  is  so  irresistible 
that  the  individual  by  "devious  and  desperate  devices"  [4]  trans- 
forms the  image  of  the  evil  father  into  the  image  of  a  good  father. 
In  this  process  of  self-deception  the  attempt  is  made  to  appease  the 
real  enemy  by  "making  suitable  concessions."  [5]  Quislingism 
thus  becomes  an  "exquisitely  homosexual"  solution.  [5]  The 
treachery  and  betrayal  associated  with  Quislingism  is  due  to  the 
fact  that  alliance  with  the  enemy  is  a  sadistic  attempt  "to  overcome 
the  incest  taboo  by  raping  the  Mother  instead  of  loving  her."  [6] 

The  psychology  of  anti-Semitism  has  been  variously  treated 
along  psychoanalytic  lines.  One  explanation  is  that  anti-Semitism 
is  an  "unconscious  hostility"  directed  to  Jews  who  are  recognized 
as  the  proponent  of  the  "monotheistic  God  (superego  God)" 


474  THE  EGO  IN  PSYCHOANALYSIS 

whose  acceptance  has  forced  the  renunciation  of  the  instincts.  [39] 
A  recent  study  of  the  personalities  of  anti-Semitic  women  con- 
cludes that  their  prejudice  is  due  to  a  very  strict  and  conventional 
superego  in  harmony  with  parental  images  so  that  instinctual 
impulses  must  find  their  outlet  in  very  devious  expressions  such  as 
anti-Semitism. 

Our  puritanical  anti-Semites  project  their  id  impulses  onto  the  Jew. 
[14,  286] 

The  solution  of  anti-Semitism,  according  to  these  authors,  there- 
fore lies  in  the  direction  of  a  more  intelligent  and  more  lenient 
child  training  on  the  part  of  the  parents  which  would  result  in 
greater  emotional  security  and  more  psychological  insight. 

An  apologist  for  a  rigid  Freudian  interpretation  of  the  develop- 
ment and  continuation  of  culture  is  found  in  Roheim.  [47] 
Although  Roheim  rejects  Freud's  contention  that  the  collective 
unconscious  can  be  inherited,  he  feels  that  all  of  man's  culture  has 
been  developed  "as  a  sublimation  or  reaction-formation  to  infantile 
conflicts."  [149]  He  substitutes  an  ontogenetic  theory  of  culture 
for  Freud's  phylogenetic  theory.  He  notes  that  the  distinctive 
feature  of  man  as  contrasted  to  other  animals  is  his  prolonged 
infancy.  This  plus  the  retarded  development  of  dentition  with 
the  consequence  that  there  is  in  the  infant  considerable  activity 
unrelated  to  the  acquisition  of  food,  together  with  the  "relatively 
precocious  sexual  development"  of  the  human  infant,  all  adds  up 
to  the  fact  that  the  long  process  of  human  maturation  and  the 
characteristics  accompanying  it  produce  a  set  of  conditions  from 
which  culture  develops.  Institutions  and  ways  of  life  not  directly 
connected  with  the  acquisition  of  food  are  regarded  as  sublima- 
tions of  the  infantile  situation.  His  view  is  "that  the  bulk  of 
human  culture,  even  in  its  adaptational  or  ego-aspects,  arises  out 
of  play  or  ritual  activities.  The  reason  for  these  activities  lies  in 
the  infantile  situation,  and  they  acquire  survival  values  secondarily 
by  assimilating  a  part  of  the  environment  to  man's  needs."  [163] 

Therefore  in  the  development  of  human  adaptations  the  tendency 
is  for  adaptations  that  are  based  on  the  infantile  situation  to  replace 
in  an  ever  decreasing  degree  those  based  on  the  stimulus-reaction 
pattern.  [47, 163] 


MODIFICATIONS  WITH  CULTURAL  EMPHASIS  475 

For  example,  according  to  Roheim,  the  art  of  making  fire  is  "a 
displaced  play  repetition  of  the  genital  act  or  of  masturbation." 
[163]  Differences  between  different  cultures  are  accounted  for  on 
the  basis  of  variations  in  the  infantile  situations  which  are  them- 
selves "probably  based  on  constitutional  variations  of  human 
groups."  Changes  that  occur  within  a  culture  are  due  to  the  fact 
that  a  culture  provides  for  the  sublimation  of  only  some  of  the  id 
trends  and  defense  mechanisms  of  the  individual  and  that  what 
tendencies  the  culture  fails  to  sublimate  provide  the  "psychic  back- 
ground" which  makes  for  reform  and  change.  But  over  the  course 
of  generations,  the  culture  adapts  itself  to  the  characteristics  of  the 
infantile  situation  so  that,  even  when  the  individual  is  affected  by 
the  institutions  of  a  culture,  these  institutions  must  be  regarded 
only  as  representing  "the  petrified  strivings  of  past  generations  of 
human  beings."  [166] 

Fenichel  is  highly  critical  of  Roheim's  ontogenetic  theory  of 
cultural  development.  He  believes  that  social  institutions,  first 
created  by  human  beings  who  were  satisfying  their  needs,  then 
become  external  realities  themselves,  part  of  the  outer  stimuli  "that 
modify  the  instinctual  structure  of  mankind."  [12,  70]  The  proc- 
ess of  socialization  for  Fenichel  is  a  reciprocal  action  where  social 
influences  modify  the  instinctual  structure  and  the  modified  in- 
stinctual structure  itself  "reacts  again  upon  social  reality."  [71] 
As  we  have  seen  in  FenichePs  account  of  the  drive  for  wealth,  he 
accepts  the  orthodox  view  concerning  man's  instinctual  structure 
and  regards  culture  and  its  institutions  essentially  as  furnishing 
objects  for  the  displacement  of  instinctual  impulses  and  their  in- 
fantile derivatives.  For  him  the  ego  is  a  direct  development  of 
the  instincts  as  they  are  influenced  by  the  outside  world.  He 
objects  to  the  psychoanalyst,  such  as  Horney,  who  "takes  from 
psycho-analysis  its  biological  basis— that  of  instinctual  needs."  [11, 
418] 

MODIFICATIONS  WITH  CULTURAL  EMPHASIS 

In  the  early  1930's  there  was  increasing  recognition  on  the  part 
of  some  analysts  of  the  cultural  variations  of  Freudian  formula- 
tions, a  sensitivity  to  the  data  and  concepts  so  rapidly  accumulating 
in  anthropology  and  ethnology.  Analysts  such  as  Fromm,  Sperber, 


476  THE  EGO  IN  PSYCHOANALYSIS 

and  Horney  took  vigorous  exception  to  some  of  Freud's  formula- 
tions which,  they  argued,  showed  a  neglect  on  Freud's  part  to  fol- 
low through  the  implications  of  the  fact  that  the  individual  devel- 
ops in  and  is  modified  by  a  social  environment  already  fairly  well 
structured  when  the  individual  is  born  into  it,  an  environment 
that  itself  has  certain  characteristics  and  peculiarities  the  psycho- 
analyst must  be  aware  of  if  his  explanations  are  to  have  any  degree 
of  universality. 

In  1927  Malinowski  published  his  Sex  and  Repression  in  Savage 
Society,  the  first  part  of  which  appeared  as  two  articles  in  1924. 
Here  he  specifically  directed  his  evidence  against  the  fundamental 
Freudian  tenet  of  the  Oedipus  complex  as  a  universal  human  phe- 
nomenon existing  independently  in  any  social  environment. 
Malinowski  based  his  argument  on  his  study  of  the  Trobriand 
Islanders  who  lived  in  a  matrilineal  society  and  who  developed  a 
matrilineal  complex  "entirely  different  in  its  genesis  and  its  char- 
acter from  the  Oedipus  complex."  [40,  83]  Malinowski  could  find 
no  evidence  among  the  Trobrianders  of  rivalry  or  friction  between 
the  father  and  the  son.  If  there  was  any  ambivalent  attitude  of 
love  and  hate  it  was  "felt  between  a  man  and  his  mother's  brother, 
while  the  repressed  sexual  attitude  of  incestuous  temptation  can  be 
formed  only  towards  his  sister."  [80] 

Malinowski's  interpretation  of  the  cultural  relativity  of  the 
Oedipus  complex  was  vigorously  attacked  by  Jones  [32]  who 
maintained  that  Malinowski's  findings  could  be  interpreted  "as  a 
mode  of  defence  against  the  primordial  Oedipus  tendencies." 
Malinowski  felt  that  orthodox  analysts  such  as  Jones  who  place 
"a  certain  causal  or  metaphysical  stress"  [40, 139]  on  the  Oedipus 
complex  as  the  cause,  rather  than  as  the  effect,  of  a  social  structure 
were  unnecessarily  divorcing  psychoanalytic  theory  from  empirical 
anthropology  which,  to  Malinowski,  clearly  shows  that  "the  nu- 
clear family  complex  is  a  functional  formation  dependent  upon 
the  structure  and  upon  the  culture  of  a  society."  [143] 

Other  anthropologists,  looking  toward  a  fruitful  and  productive 
relationship  between  psychoanalysis  and  anthropology,  urged  psy- 
choanalysts to  adapt  themselves  to  the  implications  which  the  fact 
of  cultural  variation  had  for  their  discipline.  Sapir  was  an  early 
and  outstanding  protagonist  of  this  point  of  view.  In  a  series  of 


MODIFICATIONS  WITH  CULTURAL  EMPHASIS  477 

brilliant  articles,  he  pointed  out  that  the  mechanisms  with  which 
the  psychoanalyst  was  concerned  should  not  be  regarded  as  "closed 
systems  imprisoned  within  the  biological  walls  of  isolated  indi- 
viduals" [49,  233]  and  could  be  studied  realistically  only  when 
related  to  deeply  ingrained  cultural  patterns  and  the  various  sub- 
cultures and  personal  interactions  with  other  people  that  consti- 
tute the  external  social  world  for  the  single  individual.  [48,  49,  50] 

An  early  and  particularly  vigorous  attack  on  psychoanalysis  and 
psychotherapy  in  general  for  its  neglect  of  man's  social  relation- 
ships was  made  by  Sperber  in  1932.  [53]  Sperber's  criticism  is 
from  a  Marxist  point  of  view,  and  it  is  not  without  significance 
that  the  editor  of  the  journal  in  which  his  article  appeared  felt 
obliged  to  point  out  to  the  readers  that,  although  Sperber's  com- 
ments took  on  the  nature  of  a  political  tract  and  therefore  were 
unusual  for  a  scientific  journal,  it  was  only  proper  that  a  "bour- 
geois" journal  should  show  it  could  tolerate  this  attack. 

Sperber  maintained  that  the  intellectual  efforts  of  any  given 
period  always  function  in  the  interests  of  the  ruling  class.  He  saw 
modern  psychotherapy  as  a  strictly  bourgeois  outgrowth  with  its 
emphasis  on  the  individualism  produced  by  capitalist  society  and 
its  complete  neglect  of  the  fact  that  personality  and  individual 
consciousness  are  socially  determined,  are  not  a  product  of  irra- 
tional forces  working  themselves  out  in  isolated  individuals.  Since 
life  in  the  bourgeois  capitalist  class  is  traditionally  regarded  as  a 
battle  against  competitors,  neuroses  become  inevitable:  for  the  goal 
of  individualism  is  itself  an  anachronism,  incapable  of  fulfillment. 
The  individual  can  only  be  understood,  then,  as  a  part  of  the  whole 
social  process.  In  neglecting  this,  modern  psychotherapy  has 
become  a  mystical  quest  for  drives,  not  a  real  social  psychology. 
Therapy  has  become  related  to  a  sentimental  code  of  ethics,  not 
to  any  real  understanding  of  the  need  to  socialize  the  individual, 
to  show  him  how  he  is  a  product  of  society,  to  help  him  undei> 
stand  the  particular  role  he  is  playing  in  this  transitional  period. 
It  is  no  wonder  then,  says  Sperber,  that  the  formulations  of  current 
psychotherapy,  which  approach  the  problems  of  mass  psychology 
from  an  individualistic  point  of  view,  have  failed  to  create  any 
valid  explanation  of  social  behavior,  have  substituted  in  their  stead 


478  THE  EGO  IN  PSYCHOANALYSIS 

shadowy  theories  of  a  collective  unconscious  which  separate  the 
individual  from  social  reality. 

One  of  the  most  systematic  and  closely  reasoned  revisions  of 
Freud's  formulations  to  accommodate  the  psychoanalytic  neglect 
of  social  and  cultural  influence  is  that  of  Erich  Fromm.  Begin- 
ning in  1930  with  his  account  of  the  function  of  religion,  Fromm 
has  carefully  spelled  out  what  he  regards  as  the  weaknesses  and 
blindspots  of  Freudian  theory  and  what  must  be  done  by  psycho- 
analysis to  remedy  them.  [22-26] 

Fromm  points  out  that  for  Freud  man  was  regarded  as  a  closed 
system  endowed  with  certain  drives  which  "were  looked  upon  as 
eternal  forces  rooted  in  the  biological  constitution  of  man."  [26,77] 
Although  Freud  understood  the  biological-physiological  condi- 
tions of  drive,  Fromm  feels  that  Freud  did  not  comprehend  the 
extent  to  which  drives  could  be  modified  by  social  factors.  [23,  34] 
Because  of  the  naivete  of  Freud  and  most  of  his  disciples  concern- 
ing the  actual  development  and  functioning  of  social  processes, 
Fromm  believes  that  most  of  Freud's  "applications  of  psychol- 
ogy to  social  problems  were  misleading  constructions."  [26,  9] 
Freudian  mechanisms  such  as  the  super-ego  and  the  ego,  sado- 
masochistic impulses,  Fromm  regards  not  as  givens  of  human 
nature  but  as  end  products  resulting  from  particular  social  struc- 
tures. [25,  92]  He  denies  the  universality  and  inevitable  develop- 
ment of  the  Oedipus  complex,  arguing  that  this  psychoanalytic 
construction  is  based  on  false  sociological  assumptions.  The  Oedi- 
pus complex  can  only  be  seen  as  a  development  in  a  particular 
type  of  culture.  [23,  38  /.;  25,  88]  He  notes  that  there  are  many 
societies  where  the  father  is  not  a  sexual  rival  of  the  child  or  an 
all-powerful  authority.  But  even  though  the  psychoanalytic  sup- 
position concerning  the  Oedipus  complex  is  false,  Fromm  believes 
that  this  error  should  not  be  laid  against  psychoanalytic  method 
as  such— the  essential  failure  of  psychoanalysis  was  that  it  did  not 
carry  its  methods  far  enough  and  study  the  interdependence  of  the 
individual  in  groups,  classes,  and  cultures.  [23,  38]  In  their  em- 
phasis on  the  family,  their  tendency  to  equate  the  family  to  society, 
psychoanalysts  have  failed  to  see  that  the  family  is  only  a  carrier 
of  a  particular  culture.  Hence  they  have  tended  to  identify  society 


MODIFICATIONS  WITH  CULTURAL  EMPHASIS  479 

as  a  whole  with  the  bourgeois  capitalist  family,  and  they  have 
failed  to  see  the  enormous  implications  of  their  error. 

The  psychological  make-up  of  members  of  any  group  or  any 
social  situation  can,  according  to  Fromm,  only  be  understood  if 
the  common  experiences  of  group  members  are  studied,  if  we 
know  how  the  social,  political,  or  economic  structure  of  a  society 
affects  the  individual. 

The  key  problem  of  psychology  is  that  of  the  specific  kind  of 
relatedness  of  the  individual  towards  the  world  and  not  that  of  the 
satisfaction  or  frustration  of  this  or  that  instinctual  need  per  sc. 
(26, 12] 

Particularly  important  for  Fromm  is  the  necessity  of  recognizing 
the  objective  economic  conditions  that  dominate  the  particular 
society  in  which  an  individual  lives. 

Thus  the  mode  of  life,  as  it  is  determined  for  the  individual  by  the 
peculiarity  of  an  economic  system,  becomes  the  primary  factor  in 
determining  his  whole  character  structure,  because  the  imperative 
need  for  self-preservation  forces  him  to  accept  the  conditions  under 
which  he  has  to  live.  [26, 18] 

In  one  of  his  earlier  papers  [23],  Fromm  pays  tribute  to  Marx  and 
Engels  for  pointing  out  how  objective  conditions  determine  men- 
tality. He  holds  that  the  interrelatedness  of  man  to  his  social 
environment  is  a  dialectical  process,  that  society  has  not  only  a 
suppressing  function  but  also  a  creative  function. 

Fromm  points  out  that,  since  psychoanalysis  must  itself  study 
the  conditions  of  a  society  and  the  way  they  affect  an  individual, 
it  cannot  jump  suddenly  from  individual  to  social  psychology  and 
transfer  bodily  to  social  phenomena  explanations  that  may  be  satis- 
factory in  dealing  with  certain  individual  neuroses.  He  severely 
criticizes  those  analysts  who,  reasoning  by  tenuous  analogy,  re- 
construct the  psychology  of  men  by  studying  their  ideologies  in  a 
vacuum:  we  cannot  reconstruct  men  from  their  ideology,  we  must 
learn  how  and  why  specific  ideologies  arise  as  products  of 
men.  [22]  A  particularly  prevalent  error  of  psychoanalysts, 
Fromm  notes,  arises  from  their  assumption  that  their  own  bour- 
geois society  is  "normal/'  They  have  dealt  primarily  with  neu- 
rotics within  that  class  and  have  assumed  that  the  particular 


480  THE  EGO  IN  PSYCHOANALYSIS 

background  common  to  these  bourgeois  neurotics  is  identical  to 
social  background  in  general.  [23,  35  ff.] 

Different  societies  or  classes  within  a  society  have  a  specific  social 
character,  and  on  its  basis  different  ideas  develop  and  become  power- 
ful [26,  279] 

Karen  Horney's  criticism  of  Freud  and  his  orthodox  followers 
closely  parallels  Fromm's.  In  her  several  widely  read  books  she 
has  clearly  and  ably  stated  her  qualifications  of  Freudian  theory. 
[29-31]  Freud,  she  writes,  "has  assumed  that  the  instinctual 
drives  or  object  relationships  that  are  frequent  in  our  culture  are 
biologically  determined  'human  nature'  or  arise  out  of  unalterable 
situations  (biologically  given  'pregenital'  states,  Oedipus  complex). 
Freud's  disregard  of  cultural  factors  not  only  leads  to  false  general- 
izations, but  to  a  large  extent  blocks  an  understanding  of  the  real 
forces  which  motivate  our  attitudes  and  actions.  I  believe  that  this 
disregard  is  the  main  reason  why  psychoanalysis,  inasmuch  as  it 
faithfully  follows  the  theoretical  paths  beaten  by  Freud,  seems  in 
spite  of  its  seemingly  boundless  potentialities  to  have  come  into  a 
blind  alley,  manifesting  itself  in  a  rank  growth  of  abstruse  theories 
and  the  use  of  a  shadowy  terminology."  [29,  20  /.]  She  sees 
Freud's  blindness  to  cultural  factors  "in  his  inclination  to  regard 
certain  environmental  influences  as  the  incidental  fate  of  the  indi- 
vidual instead  of  recognizing  the  whole  strength  of  cultural  influ- 
ences behind  them."  [30,  170]  And  she  notes  with  Fromm  that 
psychoanalysis  has  stemmed  from  the  study  of  middle-class  neu- 
rotics of  our  western  civilization  and  has  assumed  that  the  trends 
pervading  in  this  particular  class  in  this  society  are  biological 
factors  inherent  in  human  nature.  [30, 168  /.] 

More  recently  Abram  Kardiner,  Coordinator  of  Psychiatry  with 
the  Social  Sciences  at  Columbia  University,  has  also  rejected 
Freud's  contention  of  a  specific  and  variable  instinctual  basis 
behind  all  human  nature  but  defends  psychoanalysis  as  the  only 
technique  that  can  fruitfully  study  problems  of  personality  and 
culture.  Kardiner  believes  that  his  concept  of  basic  personality 
structure— "the  precipitate  of  the  reactions  of  the  individual  to 
specific  institutions  in  the  order  in  which  they  affect  him" — is 
the  key  to  our  understanding  of  social  problems. 


WHY  PSYCHOANALYTIC  FORMULATIONS  MUST  BE  REJECTED        481 

Freud  did  not  regard  this  concept  of  basic  personality  as  necessary 
because  it  was  assumed  to  be  the  same  in  all  human  beings  irrespec- 
tive of  culture.  [36,  484] 

The  task  of  the  psychologist  who  wants  to  understand  social 
problems  and  the  relation  of  the  individual  to  his  society  is  for 
Kardiner  that  of  discovering  the  basic  personality  structure  formed 
in  a  given  society  and  then  tracking  down  the  influences  of  this 
basic  personality  structure  on  the  culture  itself.  [36,  37] 

WHY  PSYCHOANALYTIC  FORMULATIONS  MUST  BE  REJECTED 

In  evaluating  psychoanalytic  formulations,  it  should  be  pointed 
out  that  if  we  are  to  have  a  real  science  of  psychology  we  must 
always  avoid  any  theory  or  concept  which  either  does  not  have  or 
does  not  sometime  seem  capable  of  having  a  naturalistic  explana- 
tion. The  ultimate  source  of  explanations  for  the  scientist  must 
come  from  his  knowledge  of  the  real  world.  It  cannot  be  the 
product  of  unbridled  imagination. 

From  his  Survey  of  Objective  Studies  of  Psychoanalytic  Con- 
cepts, Sears  concludes  that 

By  the  criteria  of  the  physical  sciences  it  [psychoanalysis]  is  not  a 
good  science.  .  .  .  The  experiments  and  observations  examined  in  this 
report  stand  testimony  that  few  investigators  feel  free  to  accept 
Freud's  statements  at  face  value.  The  reason  lies  in  the  same  factor 
that  makes  psychoanalysis  a  bad  science — its  method.  Psychoanalysis 
relies  upon  techniques  that  do  not  admit  of  the  repetition  of  observa- 
tion, that  have  no  self-evident  or  denotative  validity,  and  that  are 
tinctured  to  an  unknown  degree  with  the  observer's  own  suggestions. 
These  difficulties  may  not  seriously  interfere  with  therapy,  but  when 
the  method  is  used  for  uncovering  psychological  facts  that  are  required 
to  have  objective  validity  it  simply  fails.  [52, 133] 

Sears  ends  his  report  with  the  statement  "that  other  social  and 
psychological  sciences  must  gain  as  many  hypotheses  and  intui- 
tions as  possible  from  psychoanalysis  but  that  the  further  analysis 
of  psychoanalytic  concepts  by  nonpsychoanalytic  techniques  may 
be  relatively  fruitless  so  long  as  those  concepts  rest  in  the  theoretical 
framework  of  psychoanalysis."  [143] 


482  THE  EGO  IN  PSYCHOANALYSIS 

Freud's  original  formulations  have  undergone  and  are  still 
undergoing  considerable  modification  and  extension  with  the 
growing  realization  on  the  part  of  certain  analysts  of  the  impact 
of  social  realities  on  the  individual  and  the  close  interrelationship 
between  the  individual  and  the  immediate  society  around  him. 
So  psychoanalysis  at  the  present  time  is  in  a  state  of  flux  and 
development.  It  is  apparent  from  the  tone  of  many  of  the  articles 
and  reviews  appearing  in  its  journals  that  the  divergency  of  some 
members  of  the  psychoanalytic  family  from  Freud's  original  pre- 
cepts is  not  calmly  accepted.  Debate  is  often  acrimonious,  innu- 
endoes are  frequent,  and  criticism  of  one  analyst  by  another  is  not 
always  restrained.  We  mention  this  situation  here  only  because  it 
seems  to  reflect  a  fundamental  inability  to  agree  on  formulations 
which  can  be  scientifically  verified,  which  will  stand  up  on  their 
own  merits  as  demonstrable  and  unequivocal,  and  which  cannot 
be  shaken  or  reinterpreted  by  this  or  that  observation  of  a  given 
analyst. 

Simple  and  sovereign  principles.  One  of  the  noteworthy  and 
startling  impressions  left  from  a  reading  of  psychoanalytic  litera- 
ture is  the  ease  and  glibness  with  which  the  more  orthodox  analysts 
juggle  their  various  concepts  around  to  explain  quite  diversified 
phenomena.  By  appealing  to  the  same  mechanisms,  they  explain 
such  opposites  as  love  and  hate,  aggression  and  submission,  the 
development  and  function  of  contrasting  ideologies  such  as  Fascism 
and  Communism,  Catholicism  and  atheism.  They  never  seem  to 
be  stumped  in  finding  a  cause  for  any  variety  of  thought  or  be- 
havior, whether  on  the  individual  or  the  social  level.  Further- 
more, a  great  many  of  them  (persons  of  Fromm's  and  Horney's 
stature  excluded)  seem  able  to  explain  the  cause  of  social  phe^ 
nomena  without  having  to  make  detailed  and  painstaking  investi- 
gations of  the  phenomena  they  are  accounting  for.  We  saw 
previously,  for  example,  how  comparatively  easy  it  was  for  the 
psychoanalyst  to  explain  war  and  revolution,  Quislingism  and 
Fascism. 

Sometimes,  to  be  sure,  their  reasoning  seems  particularly  tortu- 
ous but  they  wind  up  in  the  end  with  the  phenomenon  to  be 
accounted  for  neatly  placed  on  top  of  the  pedestal  which  they  have 


WHY  PSYCHOANALYTIC  FORMULATIONS  MUST  BE  REJECTED        483 

built  by  putting  together  their  simple  and  sovereign  principles. 
Sapir  noted  that  the  cultural  anthropologist's  quarrel  with  psycho- 
analysis was  due  to  the  fact  that 

The  psychoanalyst  has  confused  the  archaic  in  the  conceptual  or 
theoretical  psychologic  sense  with  the  archaic  in  the  literal  chrono- 
logical sense.  .  .  .  The  service  o£  cultural  anthropology  to  psychiatry 
is  not  as  mysterious  or  remote  or  clandestine  as  psychoanalytic  mys- 
ticism would  have  us  believe.  [49,  235] 

Murphy,  Murphy,  and  Newcomb  point  out  that 

The  psychoanalyst  digs  into  the  details  of  infinitely  complex  symp- 
tom complexes  (syndromes)  and  invariably  comes  up  with  a  "cause," 
which,  just  as  invariably,  is  only  another  symptom  (another  part  of 
the  same  syndrome).  Because  these  symptoms  are  parts  of  a  syn- 
drome they  are  always  found  to  be  correlated  and  present  at  the  same 
time  or  in  a  certain  sequence;  they  are  then  thought  to  be  cause  and 
effect.  [41,  306] 

Freud  posited  certain  instincts  and  mechanisms  he  regards  as 
universal.  Without  reviewing  here  the  old  controversy  concern- 
ing instincts,  we  should  point  out,  however,  that  Freud's  various 
conceptions  of  instinct  (that  is,  death  and  destruction  and  other 
dramatic  instincts)  seem  particularly  abstract,  particularly  difficult 
ever  to  find  a  definite  locus  for  in  the  organism.  And  no  solid 
contribution  to  our  understanding  of  motivation  seems  to  be  served 
by  indulging  in  the  verbal  magic  of  creating  a  whole  host  of  needs, 
each  endowed  with  motivational  properties,  as  has  been  done  by 
Murray  and  his  followers.  [42] 

We  have  already  noted  that  evidence  from  anthropology  indi- 
cates that  Freud's  Oedipus  complex,  so  crucial  in  his  derivation  of 
the  "superego,"  can  be  regarded  only  as  a  product  of  a  particular 
set  of  cultural  conditions.  Furthermore,  various  empirical  studies 
reviewed  by  Sears  concerned  with  parent  preferences  and  other 
heterosexual  cathexes  in  our  own  culture  "cast  doubt  on  the  alleged 
universality  of  the  Oedipus  situation"  [52,  44]  and  lead  Sears  to 
the  summary  conclusion  that 

These  data  again  argue  against  any  universal  pattern  and  re-empha- 
size the  point  that  has  proved  itself  time  and  time  again  in  the  exam- 


484  THE  EGO  IN  PSYCHOANALYSIS 

ination  of  such  studies  as  these— that  the  structure  of  the  little  societies 
in  which  people  grow  up  are  too  detailed  ever  to  permit  of  the  kinds 
of  generalizations  that  Freud  has  made  concerning  the  role  of  specific 
members  of  the  family.  [43] 

Freud  has  assumed  the  Oedipus  relationship  to  exist  universally, 
and  while  other  investigators  have  found  instances  of  it,  no  indications 
of  a  universal  cross-sex  parental  preference  have  been  discovered  in 
either  children  or  adults.  [52,  134  f.] 

While  Sears  finds  that  infantile  sexuality  as  such  is  supported 
by  various  investigations,  he  writes  that 

There  is  no  evidence,  however,  to  confirm  Freud's  assumptions 
about  the  inter-relationship  between  the  various  erotogenic  zones. 
Whether  one  zone  can  serve  as  a  substitute  for  another  is  still  unde- 
termined. [52,  135] 

Sears  also  reports  from  his  survey: 

There  is  no  indication  that  the  castration  complex  is  common. 
Quite  the  contrary.  Children  whose  sex  information  is  adequate  show 
little  tendency  toward  fears  or  curious  beliefs  about  the  sexual  process. 
It  seems  probable  that  Freud's  notion  about  children's  attitudes  to- 
ward sex  were  based  on  a  small  sample  that  was  far  from  characteristic 
of  contemporary  American  children.  [52,  136] 

Furthermore,  in  chapter  9  we  saw  that  the  ideas  of  masculinity 
and  femininity  are  largely  culturally  derived  and  that  variations  in 
adult-youth  conflicts  significantly  affect  attitudes  toward  sex. 

In  spite  of  the  divergence  of  some  analysts  such  as  Fromm  and 
Horney,  who  seem  to  have  worked  out  the  most  careful  and 
cautious  reformulations  of  Freud,  and  the  great  advance  they  have 
made  in  their  formulations  by  taking  cultural  influences  into 
account,  they  have  still  by  no  means  sufficiently  emancipated  them- 
selves from  Freudian  influence  to  arrive  at  motivational  concepts 
much  less  abstract,  or  much  less  sovereign  than  Freud's. 

Fromm's  great  contribution  is  his  recognition  of  the  fact  that 
character  structure  within  a  society  develops  from  a  set  of  economic 
conditions.  But  he  accepts  a  general  Freudian  interpretation  of 
the  dynamics  of  character  as  the  only  useful  one.  [25,  80]  And  he 
subscribes  in  general  to  the  psychoanalytic  thesis  that  the  founda- 
tion of  character  is  determined  by  the  sublimation  of  drive  as  a 


WHY  PSYCHOANALYTIC  FORMULATIONS  MUST  BE  REJECTED        485 

result  of  childhood  experiences.  [24,  254]  In  brief,  Fromm  does 
not  seriously  question  Freud's  basic  formulations  or  Freudian 
mechanisms  such  as  the  libido,  masochistic  and  sadistic  strivings, 
but  he  does  feel  that  these  mechanisms  develop  as  end  products  of 
particular  social  structures;  that  they  cannot  be  regarded  merely 
as  human  endowments.  Basic  in  Fromm's  own  formulation  is  the 
concept  of  libidinal  structure,  derived  from  instinctual  energies, 
especially  sexuality.  The  particular  libidinal  structure  of  any 
individual  can  only  be  understood  with  reference  to  his  early 
conditioning  and  the  particular  pattern  of  economic  and  social 
pressures  in  the  society  in  which  he  lives.  The  libidinal  structure, 
then,  shifts  with  objective  conditions.  [23]  Because  of  the  push 
of  these  libidinal  forces,  there  is  in  everyone  a  "need  to  avoid 
aloneness"— a  need  "not  rooted  in  bodily  processes  but  in  the  very 
essence  of  the  human  mode  and  practice  of  life."  [26, 19]  Fromm 
feels  that  the  development  of  industrial  capitalism  and  the  emer- 
gence of  the  religious  freedom  of  Protestantism  have  greatly  in- 
creased the  individual  man's  sense  of  aloneness  and  isolation. 
Various  neurotic  strivings  as  well  as  social  phenomena  are,  then, 
for  Fromm  to  be  explained  as  attempts  to  escape  from  this  feeling 
of  loneliness  which  increased  freedom  brings.  Love  and  work  are 
seen  as  types  of  spontaneous  activity  which  make  it  possible  for 
the  individual  to  satisfy  his  need  to  avoid  aloneness  by  uniting 
himself  with  others.  [26,  260] 

In  accounting  for  man's  "tendency  to  grow,  to  develop  and 
realize  potentialities"  [26,  257]  Fromm  posits  two  abstract  and 
universal  motivational  mechanisms — the  striving  for  justice  and 
the  striving  for  truth. 

The  striving  for  justice  and  truth  is  an  inherent  trend  of  human 
nature,  although  it  can  be  repressed  and  perverted  like  the  striving  for 
freedom.  [26,  288] 

Fromm  recognizes  that  in  making  these  assumptions  he  is  on 
theoretically  dangerous  ground.  But  he  feels  these  drives  can  be 
accounted  for. 

Every  individual  in  childhood  goes  through  a  period  which  is 
characterized  by  powerlessness.  It  seems  to  us  that  in  this  state  of 
powerlessness  traits  like  the  sense  of  justice  and  truth  develop  and 


486  THE  EGO  IN  PSYCHOANALYSIS 

become  potentialities  common  to  man  as  such.  We  arrive  therefore 
at  the  fact  that,  although  character  development  is  shaped  by  the  basic 
conditions  of  life  and  although  there  is  no  biologically  fixed  human 
nature,  human  nature  has  a  dynamism  of  its  own  that  constitutes  an 
active  factor  in  the  evolution  of  the  social  process.  [26,  289] 

We  can  thoroughly  agree  and  applaud  Fromm's  point  that  the 
feeling  of  aloneness  characteristic  of  modern  man  is  a  by-product 
of  the  mode  and  practice  of  life.  But  the  analysis  needs  to  be 
brought  up  to  date  in  terms  of  the  complications,  differentiations, 
and  conflicts  found  in  contemporary  social  life.  For  example,  we 
saw  in  chapter  10  that  sociologists  have  accumulated  a  wealth  of 
material  dealing  with  the  marginal  position,  the  conflicting  sta- 
tuses experienced  by  many  people  today.  Furthermore,  if  such 
mechanisms  as  the  "need  to  avoid  aloneness"  and  the  Oedipus 
complex  can  be  shown  to  be  by-products  of  social  relationships, 
why  should  Fromm  not  also  apply  this  same  derivation  to  the 
strivings  for  "justice"  and  "truth"?  For  these,  too,  when  found, 
result  only  from  the  reciprocal  relationships  of  social  life.  As 
previously  noted  (pp.  334^.),  Piaget  has  shown  that  such  concepts 
of  "morality"  can  and  do  develop  spontaneously  as  a  child  begins 
to  see  its  own  place  in  a  constellation  of  social  relations. 

Horney,  too,  in  spite  of  her  reformulations,  is  weighted  down 
with  her  allegiance  to  Freud  who  has,  she  believes,  "shown  us  the 
direction  in  which  to  move."  [29,  61]  Although  highly  sensitive 
to  Freud's  lack  of  a  cultural  orientation,  she  is  so  anchored  to 
Freud's  basic  approach  that,  like  Fromm,  she  is  forced  into  the 
creation  of  new  abstractions  in  order  to  free  herself  from  Freud's 
libidinal  approach.  Her  fundamental  concept  is  that  of  "basic 
anxiety"  which  results  from  the  conflicts,  insecurities,  and  hostili- 
ties the  individual  meets  in  society,  an  anxiety  generated  by  the 
individual's  feeling  of  lonesomeness  in  a  competitive  world.  On 
the  basis  of  this  hypothesis,  she  undertakes  to  reformulate  familiar 
Freudian  mechanisms  such  as  masochism,  narcissism,  and  the 
Oedipus  complex.  Anxiety  she  sees  as  the  driving  force  behind  or 
accompanying  two  "guiding  principles"— safety  and  satisfaction— 
that  also  "rule"  man.  [30,  73]  She  only  questions  Freud's  deriva- 
tion of  these  concepts,  not  their  validity. 


WHY  PSYCHOANALYTIC  FORMULATIONS  MUST  BE  REJECTED        487 

Kardiner  accepts  uncritically  Freud's  statement  of  problems  and 
attempts  to  find  cultural  derivations  for  the  traditional  Freudian 
mechanisms.  Kardiner's  own  abstraction — basic  personality  struc- 
ture— seems,  however,  much  less  precise,  less  consistent  than 
Fromm's  need  to  avoid  isolation  or  Horney's  basic  anxiety.  Unlike 
Fromm  and  Horney,  Kardiner  does  little  more  than  pay  lip  service 
to  social  determinants.  He  resorts  to  rather  orthodox  Freudian 
mechanisms  in  accounting  for  the  basic  personality  structure  of  any 
society — the  Alorese  are  what  they  are  largely  because  "maternal 
care  is  sporadic,  inconsistent,  and  undependable,"  whereas  Plain- 
villers  are  what  they  are  largely  because  maternal  care  is  good. 
And  once  Kardiner  uncovers  some  psychoanalytic  key  by  perusal 
of  ethnological  data,  it  seems  a  relatively  simple  matter  for  him  to 
account  for  almost  any  and  all  behavior:  the  quest  for  prestige  and 
wealth,  the  genesis  and  function  of  rivalry  and  authority,  the  ap- 
peal of  ideologies. 

From  our  point  of  view,  as  we  emphasized  in  chapter  5,  a  par- 
ticularly dangerous  abstraction  is  that  of  ego  drive.  Freud's  own 
writings  are  rife  with  such  phrases  as  "the  ego  strives,"  the  "ego 
tries,"  or  "the  ego  introjects  into  itself."  Bychowsky  writes  that 

The  Ego  tries  in  vain  to  liberate  itself  from  contributions  which  it 
had  received  from  its  environment  a  long  time  ago.  [9,  324] 

Alexander  says  that 

One  main  function  of  the  ego  is  to  confront  the  facts  of  inner  per- 
ception with  the  results  of  sense  perception.  [3, 147] 

Reik  constantly  speaks  of  "ego-drives."  [46]  We  have  seen  in 
previous  chapters  that  the  ego  is  a  genetic  formation,  made  up  in 
large  part  of  social  values.  To  reify  the  ego,  endow  it  with  its  own 
drive,  is  to  indulge  in  a  psychological  redundancy  which  com- 
pletely obscures  the  real  genesis,  characteristics,  composition  of  the 
ego,  and  its  functional  role. 

Neglect  of  emergent  qualities  in  personal  and  group  interactions. 
A  fundamental  reason  why  psychoanalysis  seems  to  us  not  to 
account  in  any  realistic  way  for  the  variety  and  contrast  of  social 
phenomena,  for  the  relative  strength  and  permanency  of  some 
social  developments,  the  weakness  and  transiency  of  others,  for 


488  THE  EGO  IN  PSYCHOANALYSIS 

the  rise  of  new  norms,  for  social  change,  and  the  like,  is  due  to 
its  complete  failure  to  recognize  the  real  relationship  between  the 
individual  and  his  environment,  especially  the  fact  that  new  social 
products,  new  social  values,  as  well  as  new  individual  orientations, 
can  and  do  emerge  in  the  continuous  process  of  social  evolution. 
Social  change  can  best  be  seen  as  a  series  of  successive  stages  with 
each  new  stage  derived  from  or  heaved  up  by  what  have  proved 
to  be  the  limitations  or  contradictions  of  an  earlier  stage.  Because 
of  this  failure  to  recognize  that  emergence  can  and  does  take  place 
on  the  psychological  and  social  level  as  well  as  on  the  physical  and 
biological  level,  analysts  have  been  forced  to  create  their  various 
abstractions  and  to  account  for  individual  and  social  behavior 
with  circuitous  derivations.  Indeed,  as  we  pointed  out  at  the  very 
beginning  of  this  chapter,  the  problems  of  the  neurotic  with  whom 
psychoanalysts  are  primarily  concerned,  as  well  as  psychoanalytic 
formulations  themselves,  can  be  seen  as  emergents  of  a  particular 
set  of  conditions  that  obtain  in  a  particular  form  of  society  at  a 
particular  point  in  man's  history. 

Freud  himself  minimized  this  fact  of  the  emergence  of  new 
qualities  with  new  characteristics  and  explained  what  he  termed 
"apparently  new  characteristics"  as  only  fresh  manifestations  of 
unconscious  mechanisms. 

From  our  point  of  view  we  need  not  attribute  so  much  importance 
to  the  appearance  of  new  characteristics.  For  us  it  would  be  enough 
to  say  that  in  a  group  the  individual  is  brought  under  conditions 
which  allow  him  to  throw  off  the  repressions  of  his  unconscious  in- 
stincts. The  apparently  new  characteristics  which  he  then  displays 
arc  in  fact  the  manifestations  of  this  unconscious,  in  which  all  that  is 
evil  in  the  human  mind  is  contained  as  a  predisposition.  [18,  9/., 
italics  ours] 

In  this  statement  Freud  not  only  neglects  the  mutual  dependence 
and  die  reciprocal  relationship  between  the  individual  and  other 
individuals  that  constitute  the  group;  he  neglects  too  the  facts  of 
cognition  and  perception  which,  as  we  saw  in  previous  chapters, 
unequivocally  demonstrate  the  relatedness  of  one  stimulus  to 
another,  of  one  experience  to  another. 


WHY  PSYCHOANALYTIC  FORMULATIONS  MUST  BE  REJECTED        489 

For  Freud,  the  whole  psychology  of  judgment  is  regarded  essen- 
tially as  a 

.  .  .  derivation  of  an  intellectual  function  from  the  interplay  of  the 
primary  instinctual  impulses.  Judging  has  been  systematically  de- 
veloped out  of  what  was  in  the  first  instance  introduction  into  the  ego 
and  expulsion  from  the  ego  carried  out  according  to  the  pleasure- 
principle.  Its  polarity  appears  to  correspond  to  the  opposition  between 
the  two  groups  of  instincts  which  we  have  assumed  to  exist.  Affirma- 
tion, as  being  a  substitute  for  union,  belongs  to  Eros;  while  negation, 
the  derivative  of  expulsion,  belongs  to  the  instincts  of  destruction. 
[20,  370] 

Although  the  relationship  between  the  individual  and  his  social 
environment  has  been  explicitly  recognized  by  analysts  such  as 
Fromm  and  Fenichel,  their  failure  to  take  into  sufficient  account 
the  characteristics  of  stimulus  situations  (relatedness,  degree  of 
structuration),  their  failure  to  recognize  the  full  implications  of 
the  fact  that  well-structured  situations  can  and  do  develop  cogni- 
tive frames  for  the  individual,  together  with  their  failure  to  take 
full  account  of  the  consequences  that  arise  when  old  frames  prove 
inadequate  to  account  meaningfully  for  new  objective  conditions 
with  the  consequent  high  suggestibility  of  the  individual  faced 
with  an  ambiguous  stimulus,  all  boil  down  to  the  generalization 
that  even  those  reformulators  of  Freud  who  are  anxious  to  give  due 
place  to  social  influences  are  either  neglectful  of  or  ignorant  of 
established  facts  in  the  psychology  of  perception.  Fenichel  writes, 
for  example, 

The  statement  that  the  production  and  dissemination  of  the  ideol- 
ogy of  a  society  must  be  understood  from  the  actual  economic  condi- 
tions of  this  society,  the  "super-structure"  of  which  is  the  ideology; 
that  further  they  are  to  be  understood  from  the  fact  that  this  "super- 
structure," by  means  of  the  actions  of  human  beings,  reacts  back  again 
upon  the  "foundation,"  the  economic  conditions  modifying  them — 
these  statements  are  correct  but  general.  They  become  more  specific 
when  we  succeed  in  comprehending  scientifically  the  details  of  the 
mechanisms  of  these  transiformations,  and  only  psychoanalysis  is  able 
to  help  us  in  that.  [12,  94] 


490  THE  EGO  IN  PSYCHOANALYSIS 

Without  denying  in  any  way  the  tremendous  insight  Freud  has 
provided  us  for  the  understanding  of  unconscious  motivation,  it  is 
our  contention  that  these  "more  specific"  and  more  scientific 
details  Fenichel  quite  properly  feels  are  necessary,  can  only  be 
provided  when  the  psychology  of  cognition,  together  with  a 
realistic  psychology  of  motivation,  are  brought  together. 

It  is  instructive,  in  this  regard,  to  see  how  the  psychology  of 
perception  and  cognition  is  handled  by  Kardiner.  It  is  clear  from 
the  preface  of  his  first  volume  [36]  that  Kardiner  equates  psy- 
chology with  psychiatry  or  psychoanalysis  and  that  in  his  cursory 
review  of  psychological  approaches  and  his  defense  of  psycho- 
analysis later  on  in  the  book  he  specifically  states  "we  are  omitting 
the  experimental  psychologies."  [356]  On  page  1  of  his  first  book 
devoted  to  his  basic  orientation,  he  writes : 

A  psychology  which  elects  to  study  the  cognitive  and  apperceptive 
functions  of  man  does  not  make  contact  at  any  point  with  sociology. 
[36] 

This  statement  is,  at  best,  plausible  only  if  we  accept  as  "psychol- 
ogy" a  narrow  19th  century  variety.  Any  modern  elementary 
textbook  of  psychology  will  show  that  cognition  and  apperception 
cannot  be  thought  of  as  distinct  and  isolated  processes  somehow 
insulated  from  aflfectivity  and  emotion  or  that  somehow  occur 
independent  of  temperament  or  of  motivational  factors,  many  of 
which  are  socially  determined.  We  can  refer  back  here  to  our 
discussion  of  F.  C.  Bartlett's  impressive  experiments  (pp.  32  ff.) 
and  repeat  again  his  conclusion  that 

It  [the  perceptual  reaction]  is  directed  by  interest  and  by  feeling, 
and  may  be  dominated  by  certain  crucial  features  of  the  objects  and 
scenes  dealt  with.  [6,  31] 

On  the  first  page  of  his  book  Kardiner  also  says: 

On  the  other  hand,  a  sociology  whose  chief  concern  is  to  correlate 
the  phenomena  resulting  from  the  behavior  of  human  beings  in 
groups  does  not  need  psychology.  [36] 

This  would  seem  to  mean,  then,  that  for  Kardiner  the  whole 
problem  of  frame  formation  and  the  effects  of  frame  formation  on 


WHY  PSYCHOANALYTIC  FORMULATIONS  MUST  BE  REJECTED        491 

perception  and  judgment  are  irrelevant.  Yet  Kardiner  himself, 
in  the  final  chapter  of  this  book  dealing  with  principles  and 
techniques,  is  forced  to  include  as  one  of  the  major  elements  of 
personality  "perception:  of  the  outer  world,  of  oneself,  and  of 
oneself  in  relation  to  the  outer  world."  [461]  And  he  further 
states  that 

The  complicated  forms  of  attitudinized  perceptions  both  of  other 
individuals  and  of  oneself  are  the  most  important  for  our  considera- 
tion. [36,  463] 

In  his  second  volume  Kardiner  writes  that 

Direct  experience  involves  all  the  coordinative  functions,  such  as 
discrimination  and  judgment.  [37,  8] 

And  he  uses  as  the  only  source  of  data  other  than  his  "psycho- 
dynamic"  approach  the  Rorschach  technique,  a  technique  wholly 
designed  to  study  apperceptive  functions.  Thus  Kardiner  bows 
to  perception  and  cognition  as  important  psychological  processes 
to  be  taken  into  account,  employs  enthusiastically  a  technique  to 
study  perception,  but  deliberately  omits  any  examination  of  the 
rich  literature  on  perception  found  in  experimental  psychology. 
It  is  no  wonder  that  he  fails  completely  to  see  the  relevance  the 
psychology  of  perception  has  for  any  valid  and  systematic  account 
of  the  relationships  that  he  himself  has  posed  as  the  objects  of 
investigation. 

We  have  already  shown  how  the  first  stage  in  the  actual  forma- 
tion of  an  attitude  is  a  perceptual  stage,  how  an  individual  can 
and  does  evolve  some  frame  of  reference  when  repeatedly  faced 
with  a  stimulus  situation,  how  the  structuredness  of  a  stimulus 
situation  must  be  regarded  as  a  gradation  where  the  less  ambigu- 
ous the  stimulus  situation  itself  is,  the  more  likely  is  it  to  compel 
the  formation  of  a  frame.  On  the  other  hand,  the  more  ambiguous 
the  stimulus  situation  itself  is,  the  more  likely  is  the  frame  to  be 
generated  by  internal  and  internalized  factors.  And  in  addition 
to  the  gradation  of  structuredness  in  the  stimulus  field,  the  selec- 
tivity of  perception  is  often  determined  by  the  organism's  in- 
stinctual tensions  and  by  other  motivational  and  affective  factors 
as  was  stressed  in  earlier  chapters. 


492  THE  EGO  IN  PSYCHOANALYSIS 

The  fact  that  the  individual's  perception  of  new  relationships 
between  himself  and  the  environment  does  cause  the  emergence 
of  new,  perhaps  "uncharacteristic,"  reactions  has  been  observed  by 
Murphy,  Murphy,  and  Newcomb  in  their  review  of  experimental 
social  psychology.  They  write: 

It  is  all  too  often  forgotten  that  the  condition  of  general  arousal  or 
excitement  brought  about  by  any  object  may  not  only  permit  new 
forms  of  experience  but  tap  new  drives,  leading  to  intense  activity  in 
reference  to  the  object  utterly  different  from  that  prevailing  the 
moment  before.  [41, 105] 

We  have  already  discussed  in  some  detail  the  outstanding  studies 
of  Piaget  on  the  development  of  a  constellation  of  reciprocal  rela- 
tions in  which  an  individual  locates  himself  psychologically,  (pp. 
334  #.)  Piaget's  conclusions  must  be  mentioned  again  here  since 
they  bear  so  pointedly  on  the  psychoanalytic  conception  of  the 
development  of  the  "superego."  As  the  result  of  painstaking  inves- 
tigation on  hundreds  of  cases,  Piaget  showed  that  the  first  stage  in 
the  development  of  the  child's  morality  was,  indeed,  a  result  of 
his  identification  with  grown-ups  and  his  acceptance  of  their 
standards  as  the  criteria  of  what  is  right  and  wrong — the  stage  of 
moral  realism.  However— and  this  is  the  important  part  of  the 
story  for  psychoanalysis — the  child's  morality  does  not  remain  on 
this  level  alone.  It  merges,  rather,  into  what  Piaget  regards  as  a 
different  stage  where  rules  and  regulations  lose  their  reified  reality 
and  where  right  and  wrong  are  judged  in  terms  of  a  feeling  of 
reciprocity  that  develops  under  the  influence  of  group  situations. 
From  this  stage  there  emerges  still  another  stage  where  increasing 
maturity  and  further  social  relationships  developed  in  Piaget's 
children  a  sense  of  equity.  What  is  happening  here  is  that  the 
original  frames  derived  from  the  parents  sooner  or  later  prove 
themselves  inadequate  for  any  satisfactory  adjustment  to  the  group 
situation.  These  frames  are,  then,  at  least  in  part,  discarded  in 
favor  of  new  standards  imposed  on  the  individual  by  his  relation- 
ship to  others  in  the  group. 

Piaget's  studies  are  exceedingly  well  documented  and,  within 
the  framework  of  the  culture  in  which  they  were  made,  exceed- 
ingly convincing.  Norms  that  arise  in  the  group  situation  become 


WHY  PSYCHOANALYTIC  FORMULATIONS  MUST  BE  REJECTED        493 

interiorized  in  the  individual  as  his  standards:  they  become  part 
of  his  ego.  These  results  of  Piaget,  together  with  the  data  from 
cultural  anthropology  which  show  the  extreme  variation  of  the 
Oedipus  complex  even  within  a  single  culture,  indicate  that  the 
psychoanalytic  concept  of  the  universal  development  of  a  "super- 
ego" in  human  beings  has  no  basis  in  fact  as  a  special  part  of  the 
ego  with  a  special  function. 

It  is  significant  that,  although  Piaget's  studies  are  extremely 
well  known  and  generally  highly  regarded  by  psychologists,  they 
are  very  seldom  referred  to  by  psychoanalysts.  Analysts  are  either 
ignorant  of  Piaget's  work  or  else  refuse  to  come  to  grips  with  its 
implications.  Either  state  of  affairs  is  inexcusable  in  a  discipline 
that  has  scientific  pretensions. 

We  have  seen  that  one  of  the  inevitable  psychological  emergents 
of  an  individual's  participation  in  a  group  is  his  group  conscious- 
ness, his  awareness  of  his  relationship  to  the  group,  and  the  modi- 
fication of  his  own  frames  or  attitudes  by  group  norms  (ch.  10). 
We  have  seen  how  the  individual  becomes  extremely  suggestible 
to  new  formulations,  new  orientations,  new  anchorages  if  and 
when  he  becomes  aware  of  a  wide  disparity  between  his  old  frames 
and  new  situations  or  if  he  is  confronted  with  new  and  unstruc- 
tured situations  which  none  of  his  established  frames  give  meaning 
to  or  adequately  serve  as  a  readiness  for  response.  Change  is  one 
of  the  outstanding  characteristics  of  the  world  in  which  men  live; 
sometimes  change  follows  long  periods  of  comparative  stability; 
sometimes  it  is  violent,  sometimes  relatively  gradual.  New  inven- 
tions, new  technological  developments,  new  discoveries  of  nature's 
resources  and  secrets  continually  affect  and  bring  into  new  relation- 
ships the  external  stimuli  that  constitute  man's  environment. 
Under  the  impact  of  such  events  and  occurrences,  old  frames  fre- 
quently prove  inadequate  for  interpretation  or  adjustment:  new 
frames  take  their  place  or  modify  them,  new  frames  formed  either 
as  the  result  of  the  perception  of  well-structured  stimulus  situations 
or  as  the  result  of  the  acceptance  of  new  norms,  new  orientations 
provided  by  a  leader  or  emerging  as  a  product  of  social  inter- 
course. As  we  have  said  earlier,  these  new  formulations  either 
may  or  may  not  account  objectively  for  the  new  situations.  What 
happens  when  new  formulations  do  not  adequately  account  for 


494  THE  EGO  IN  PSYCHOANALYSIS 

new  situations  cannot  be  gone  into  here.  Nor  can  we  consider  in 
detail  now  the  psychological  conditions  that  give  rise  to  new 
norms.  Here  we  are  only  pointing  out  the  fact  that  new  norms 
do  emerge,  that  in  critical  times  the  role  of  leadership  as  a  source 
of  new  orientations  is  accentuated,  that  the  individual  is  by  no 
means  always  unaware  of  changes  taking  place  in  his  external 
world. 

A  typical  explanation  of  this  process  of  social  change  by  an 
outstanding  orthodox  Freudian  psychoanalyst,  Ernest  Jones,  is  as 
follows: 

The  outstanding  discovery  of  psycho-analysis  in  this  context  [evo- 
lution and  creative  change]  has  been  that  many — and  some  analysts 
would  be  inclined  to  say  all — of  the  original  discoveries  and  better- 
ments and  improvements  of  all  kinds  that  previously  were  attributed 
to  the  action  of  purely  creative  impulses  arc  rather  to  be  regarded  as 
by-products  resulting  from  the  action  of  certain  defensive  mental 
mechanisms.  The  endeavor  to  escape  from  unconscious  guilt  and 
anxiety  leads  to  infinitely  varied  mental  activities,  some  of  which 
produce  what  may  socially  be  called  "improvements."  To  the  ideal- 
ism and  self-esteem  of  mankind  it  is  a  chastening  reflection  that  so 
much  of  what  he  is  most  proud  of  is  merely  an  accidental  result  of 
the  flight  from  fear  and  painf  that  a  bad  conscience  should  prove  to 
be  one  of  the  prime  motors  in  even  our  loftiest  strivings.  [35,  203, 
italics  ours] 

There  is  here  a  complete  neglect  of  the  fact  of  emergence  on  the 
social  level  and  of  demonstrable  psychological  principles  that  at 
least  in  part  explain  this  emergence.  It  is  no  wonder  then  that  the 
only  advice  Jones  can  give  to  speed  human  progress  is  to  improve 
the  "libidinal"  basis  of  human  nature. 

As  a  specific  example  of  the  consequences  of  the  failure  of 
psychoanalysts  to  recognize  the  emergence  of  new  products  under 
new  conditions,  we  might  refer  back  to  Jones's  account  of  the 
appeal  of  revolutionary  leaders  during  critical  times.  It  will  be 
remembered  that  leaders  arise  in  critical  situations,  according  to 
Jones,  because  the  latent  sense  of  guilt  all  human  beings  have  is 
"stirred  by  any  great  misfortune  or  privation."  [206]  Without 
seeing  the  full  implications  of  what  it  means  for  a  human  being 
to  be  "stirred  by  any  great  misfortune  or  privation,"  Jones  tells  us 


WHY  PSYCHOANALYTIC  FORMULATIONS  MUST  BE  REJECTED        495 

that  revolutionary  leaders  have  homosexual  tendencies  which  re- 
sult in  a  paranoid  defense,  and  as  a  consequence  they  have  no 
compunctions  against  violence  (which  for  them  is  the  murder  of 
the  father). 

We  reach  thus  the  conclusion  that  a  successful  revolutionary  must 
be  more  than  a  little  mad.  [35,  200] 

By  identifying  himself  with  the  leader,  substituting  the  concept 
of  society  for  the  image  of  the  parent,  the  individual  who  takes 
part  in  a  revolution  is  explained.  While  it  may,  to  be  sure,  be  true 
that  certain  leaders  during  critical  times  such  as  Hitler,  Roehm, 
and  Goering  had  homosexual  tendencies,  it  would  seem  to  be 
stretching  the  argument  beyond  the  breaking  point  to  account  for 
the  leadership  of  such  men  as  Washington,  Jefferson,  Lincoln, 
Lenin,  Stalin,  or  Franklin  Roosevelt  on  similar  grounds.  As 
J.  B.  S.  Haldane  has  said: 

Hitler  may  well  be  a  neurotic.  But  it  is  only  in  particular  circum- 
stances that  a  great  people  chooses  a  neurotic  of  this  kind  as  its  leader. 
[28, 15] 

There  seems  little  doubt  that,  if  Hitler  and  his  satellites  had  never 
been  born,  under  these  same  circumstances  other  leaders  would 
have  arisen  in  Germany  who  may  or  may  not  have  been  free  of 
homosexual  suspicions. 

Failure  to  recognize  full  implications  of  an  individual's  place- 
ment in  a  culture.  We  repeat  again  that  social  norms — institu- 
tional controls,  symbols  of  authority,  social  values — are  all  first 
external  to  the  individual  in  any  culture.  Among  these  norms 
are  the  ethical  codes  of  society  or  of  a  group  which  do  seem  in 
most  cultures  to  be  transmitted  by  the  parents.  In  reviewing  the 
learning  process  in  social  situations,  Murphy,  Murphy,  and  New- 
comb  describe  the  situation  as  follows: 

On  the  whole,  most  values  seem  to  be  "taken"  and  used  by  the 
child  because  of  a  positive  response  to  those  whose  values  come  within 
his  ken.  The  process  by  which  many  values  are  transmitted  from  one 
generation  to  the  next  is  largely  a  process,  we  believe,  involving  rela- 
tively little  coercion,  relatively  little  Freudian  repression.  It  is  more 
of  the  character  of  the  "Look,  see!"  pattern.  The  world  is  so  full  of  a 


496  THE  EGO  IN  PSYCHOANALYSIS 

number  of  things  both  lovely  and  hateful  that,  unless  the  child's  in- 
terests are  in  gross  conflict  with  the  adult,  the  parent  has  relatively 
little  trouble  in  enabling  the  child  to  see  why  one  game  is  fun  for 
three-year-olds  and  another  is  dangerous  for  three-year-olds.  Even 
without  benefit  of  identification,  which,  of  course,  plays  its  powerful 
part  in  forming  values,  the  parent  helps  the  child  to  structure  the 
world  in  terms  of  his  own  values  simply  by  pointing  out  the  palpable 
and  inherent  goods  and  bads.  [41,  238  /.] 

As  we  have  mentioned,  Piaget's  studies  unequivocally  show  that 
in  the  first  stage  of  morality  the  child  accepts  as  good  or  bad,  as 
fair  or  unfair,  the  types  of  behavior  so  judged  by  his  parents  or  by 
other  adults  who  constitute  his  immediate  social  world  (code  of 
heteronomy).  But  there  is  for  the  individual  no  psychological 
difference  either  in  the  genesis  of  or  the  function  of  "moral"  codes 
which  psychoanalysts  separate  out  as  the  "superego"  and  other 
norms  of  behavior  the  individual  learns.  The  emerging  develop- 
ing ego  is  in  large  part  composed  of  all  these  interiorized  social 
values. 

The  important  question,  the  question  psychoanalysts  have  not 
squarely  faced,  is  the  question  of  what  norms  arc  interiorized  by  a 
given  individual  in  a  society.  For  it  should  be  clear  that  by  no 
means  all  persons  in  a  given  "society"  are  exposed  to  the  same 
standards,  values,  or  norms,  except,  perhaps,  in  extremely  primi- 
tive and  small  societies.  And,  even  if  exposed  to  the  same  norms, 
individuals  acquire  ego-attitudes  only  in  relation  to  certain  refer- 
ence or  membership  groups.  As  Sapir  has  pointed  out: 

The  culture  so  carefully  described  in  our  ethnological  and  sociologi- 
cal monographs  are  not,  and  cannot  be,  the  truly  objective  entities 
they  claim  to  be.  No  matter  how  accurate  their  individual  itemiza- 
tion,  their  integrations  into  suggested  structures  are  uniformly  falla- 
cious and  unreal.  [50,  411] 

The  psychologist,  then,  who  speaks  glibly  of  a  "culture"  or 
"society"  is  speaking  of  an  abstraction. 

The  term  "society"  is  itself  a  cultural  construct  which  is  employed 
by  individuals  who  stand  in  significant  relations  to  each  other  in  order 
to  help  them  in  the  interpretation  of  certain  aspects  of  their  behavior. 
The  true  locus  of  culture  is  in  the  interactions  of  specific  individuals 


WHY  PSYCHOANALYTIC  FORMULATIONS  MUST  BE  REJECTED        497 

and,  on  the  subjective  side,  in  the  world  of  meanings  which  each  one 
of  these  individuals  may  unconsciously  abstract  for  himself  from  his 
participation  in  these  interactions.  Every  individual  is,  then,  in  a 
very  real  sense,  a  representative  of  at  least  one  sub-culture  which  may 
be  abstracted  from  the  generalized  culture  of  the  group  of  which  he 
is  a  member.  Frequently,  if  not  typically,  he  is  a  representative  of 
more  than  one  sub-culture,  and  the  degree  to  which  the  socialized 
behavior  of  any  given  individual  can  be  identified  with  or  abstracted 
from  the  typical  or  generalized  culture  of  a  single  group  varies 
enormously  from  person  to  person.  [49,  235  f.] 

This  warning  of  Sapir's  becomes  increasingly  important  when  the 
psychologist  is  concerned  with  the  study  of  individuals  who  live 
in  complex  societies,  characterized  by  definite  class,  status,  racial, 
or  interest  group  relationships.  If  we  are  to  understand  the  full 
force  of  the  social  environment  on  the  psychological  makeup  of 
the  individual,  we  must  analyze  precisely  the  interconnections  be- 
tween the  individual  and  the  social  stimuli  he  is  confronted  with. 
And  we  must  furthermore  constantly  bear  in  mind  that  the  social 
environment  is  seldom,  if  ever,  completely  static,  that  it  is,  espe- 
cially these  days,  in  a  process  of  flux  and  change,  due  to  the  impact 
of  new  technological  developments,  new  discoveries,  and  new 
inventions. 

When  psychoanalysts  use  the  concept  of  "culture"  they  seem  to 
equate  it  either  with  intrapersonal  (such  as  familial,  parent-child 
relationships)  or  with  broad  "cultural  patterns"  transmitted  to  the 
child  through  the  family.  There  is  a  general  neglect  of  different 
class,  status,  racial,  or  group  relationships  which  have  their  own 
characteristic  standards  or  norms.  As  a  consequence  of  this  failure 
to  give  sufficient  emphasis  to  an  individual's  identification  with  a 
certain  class,  status,  or  group,  there  follows  the  failure  to  recognize 
the  important  fact  that  individuals  can  and  do  seek  to  satisfy  their 
own  egos  through  the  preservation  of  or  the  enhancement  of  a 
larger  class,  status,  or  group  interest.  And  this  implies  that  indi- 
viduals can  be  and  are  conscious  of  their  class,  status,  or  group 
affiliation  and  identification,  that  this  consciousness  of  oneself  as 
a  part  of  a  larger  social  unit  can  be  and  is  a  relative  matter,  a 
consciousness  of  identification  or  belongingness  that  will  vary  in 
different  situations  and  develop  according  to  different  conditions. 


498  THE  EGO  IN  PSYCHOANALYSIS 

The  relative  degree  to  which  different  groups  of  people  are  con- 
scious of  their  group  belongingness  is  given  little  attention  by 
psychoanalysts  who,  if  they  recognize  group  or  class  problems  at 
all,  generally  assume  that  members  of  a  group  behave  uncon- 
sciously and  rather  automatically  as  such. 

In  a  penetrating  analysis  of  the  cultural  orientations  of  Fromm 
and  Horney,  Green  has  pointed  out  that  "no  individual  participates 
in  the  total  cultural  complex  totally."  [27,  534] 

Complex  cultures  do  not  lend  themselves  easily  to  the  explaining  of 
behavior  in  general-cultural  terms.  Since  individuals  interact  within 
a  small  segment  of  a  differentiated  society  and  are  inculcated  with  the 
specialized  values  of  their  various  segments  as  well  as  with  the  general- 
cultural  values,  extreme  caution  is  required  in  the  use  of  such  a 
concept  as  "modern  man."  [27,  536] 

We  can  illustrate  the  use  of  "culture"  in  a  relatively  abstract 
sense  with  reference  to  the  formulations  of  Fromm  and  Horney 
who,  as  we  have  seen,  compared  to  the  bulk  of  psychoanalysts,  are 
distinguished  for  their  attempt  to  put  psychoanalytic  theory  in  a 
cultural  framework.  Fromm,  for  example,  speaks  of  the  "social 
character"  of  a  people  in  a  society  and  equates  this  with  majority 
characteristics.  [26,  283]  In  discussing  education  as  a  mechanism 
for  transmitting  the  social  character  of  society,  he  points  out  that 
the  family  "may  be  considered  to  be  the  psychological  agent  of 
society."  [257]  But  he  does  not  give  due  emphasis  to  the  enor- 
mously different  types  of  family  relationships  found  in  our  society 
and  the  widely  differing  values  that  may  be  transmitted  by  differ- 
ent families,  as,  for  example,  the  values  transmitted  to  a  child  in 
the  family  of  a  Wall  Street  banker  contrasted  to  those  transmitted 
in  the  family  of  a  Pittsburgh  steel  worker  or  an  Alabama  Negro 
sharecropper.  Another  example  of  the  tremendous  variations 
overlooked  within  our  own  culture  by  Fromm  is  reflected  in  his 
statement  that  "all  normal  persons  today  have  about  the  same 
impulse  to  work"  [252] — surely  a  questionable  thesis  if  one  com- 
pares, for  example,  the  motivation  of  a  highly  educated  and  com- 
petent young  engineer  with  a  career  ahead  of  him  and  a  young 
man  whose  education  stopped  after  the  first  year  of  high  school, 
who  has  no  special  skills,  mediocre  abilities,  and  who  is  looking 


WHY  PSYCHOANALYTIC  FORMULATIONS  MUST  BE  REJECTED     49$ 

for  any  kind  of  job  that  will  enable  him  to  make  ends  meet.  And 
as  Green  indicates,  this  "impulse  to  work"  posited  by  Fromm  may 
be  expected  to  undergo  radical  changes  "in  a  period  of  contracting 
capitalism,  with  increasing  emphasis  upon  consumption  outlets 
and  state-guaranteed  economic  security  rather  than  upon  expand- 
ing production,  saving,  and  individual  responsibility."  [27,  536] 
Speaking  of  modern  man's  freedoms,  Fromm  says: 

He  has  become  free  from  the  external  bonds  that  would  prevent 
him  from  doing  and  thinking  as  he  sees  fit.  He  would  be  free  to  act 
according  to  his  own  will,  if  he  knew  what  he  wanted,  thought,  and 
felt.  But  he  does  not  know.  [26,  255] 

This  statement,  too,  appears  unrealistic  when  one  glances,  for 
example,  at  newspaper  accounts  of  industrial  strife  where  both 
labor  and  management  seem  very  definitely  to  know  what  they 
want  or  when  one  reads  in  the  same  newspaper  of  efforts  to  create 
legislation  to  prevent  job  discrimination  for  reasons  of  racial  back- 
ground. 

Horney  sees  basic  anxiety  as  a  driving  force  that  leads  to  a 
craving  for  affection  and  for  power  on  the  part  of  the  individual 
and  says  that  "craving  for  affection  may  be  restricted  to  certain 
groups  of  persons,  perhaps  to  one  with  which  there  are  interests  in 
common,  such  as  a  political  or  religious  group."  [29,  118]  It 
seems  to  us  that  Horney  has  the  cart  before  the  horse,  except  in 
the  cases  of  certain  isolated  individuals.  The  evidence  we  have 
reviewed  in  earlier  chapters  indicates  that  the  values,  standards,  or 
norms  of  a  group  which  become  interiorized  in  the  individual  as 
his  loyalty  structure  are  not  generally  due  to  a  basic  anxiety  under- 
lying a  craving  for  affection  which  is  then  simply  attached  to  a 
group  because  of  common  interests,  but,  rather,  that  these  loyalty 
structures  develop,  are  learned  because  of  an  individual's  placement 
in  the  social  context  due  to  the  accident  of  birth,  the  role  he  has  as 
employer  or  employed,  the  type  of  work  he  is  engaged  in  or  some 
other  circumstances  that  define  his  social  role. 

We  should  perhaps  in  passing  also  mention  the  abstract  use 
Kardiner  makes  of  culture.  The  basic  personality  structure  in  any 
society  is,  he  holds,  derived  from  influences  of  primary  institutions 


500  THE  EGO  IN  PSYCHOANALYSIS 

and  is'  dependent  on  rather  identical  influences.  For  example, 
Kardiner  says  that  "good  maternal  care  is  a  feature  of  our  cul- 
ture/' [37,  20]  Deviations  from  this  supposed  common  cultural 
feature  are  passed  off  as  atypical  or  accidental.  Although  Kardiner 
does  notice  that  "in  studying  basic  personality  structure  in  our 
culture,  new  differentiations  must  be  made  not  only  for  sex  and 
age,  but  especially  for  status,"  [38]  this  important  confession  does 
not  force  any  modification  of  Kardiner's  concept  of  the  ego  or 
basic  personality  structure.  Status  differences,  no  matter  how 
great,  are  rationalized  as  different  manifestations  of  the  same  basic 
personality  structure: 

A  king  and  a  commoner  may  be  on  opposite  sides  of  a  certain 
situation  with  regard  to  subsistence  or  prestige.  But  the  roles  of  both 
are  merely  different  facets  of  the  same  ego  structure  .  .  .  both  are 
reacting  from  different  polarities  to  the  same  ego  situation.  [36,  132] 

That  Kardiner's  formulation  is  both  inadequate  and  invalid  is  seen 
particularly  clearly  in  his  discussion  of  "Plainville,"  a  modern 
community  in  the  United  States.  He  notes  that  "the  heterogeneity 
of  our  culture  becomes  a  troublesome  question  wherever  we  dis- 
cover that  within  the  same  range  of  basic  personality  structure 
differences  in  character  become  involved  with  serious  mtra-social 
tensions  and  clashes  of  interest."  [37,  339]  But,  he  continues,  "the 
heterogeneity  of  our  culture  in  no  way  invalidates  the  attempt 
to  establish  the  general  characteristics  of  Western  man."  [339] 
Kardiner  minimizes  these  troublesome  questions  and  dismisses  the 
problems  raised  through  cultural  diversity  by  creating  "charac- 
terological  types."  [345] 

Because  the  psychoanalytic  formulations  fail  to  take  into  full 
account  the  implications  of  social  stratification  they  also  fail,  as 
Francis  Bartlett  has  forcefully  pointed  out,  to  take  into  account  the 
fact  that  individuals  within  our  society  can  and  do  develop  alle- 
giances to  social  values  that  are  themselves  conflicting  and  con- 
tradictory and  that  individuals  can  and  do  become  sensitive  to 
contradictions  between  established  social  values  and  everyday  life 
experiences.  Bartlett  illustrates  his  point  with  the  following 
schematic  example: 


WHY  PSYCHOANALYTIC  FORMULATIONS  MUST  BE  REJECTED        501 

Imagine  that  toward  the  end  of  the  depression  an  auto  worker  gets 
a  job  as  foreman  in  a  plant.  He  identifies  himself  with  his  men, 
works  side  by  side  with  them  from  time  to  time,  knows  some  of  them 
socially  and  treats  them  as  equals.  At  the  same  time,  he  has  a  new 
authority  over  them,  must  order  them  to  do  what  the  employer 
requires  and  can  fire  them  if  they  refuse.  He  begins  to  find  that  the 
exertion  of  his  authority  in  the  employer's  interests  threatens  his 
friendships.  And  his  identification  with  the  interests  of  his  men 
threatens  his  ambition  to  get  ahead  and  might  cost  him  his  job.  Here 
is  a  simple  case  of  the  contradictions  inherent  in  a  middle-class  posi- 
tion. It  is  an  unstable  situation.  The  foremen  have  not  yet  organized 
their  own  union,  and  a  fraternal  bond  with  the  U.A.W.  has  not  been 
established.  Our  foreman  may  in  time  begin  to  develop  the  feeling 
that  the  other  men  are  not  his  equals,  partly  because  he  attributes  his 
advancement  to  his  own  superiority  and  partly  because  the  men, 
whose  interests  are  no  longer  identical  with  his,  may  behave  in  what 
seems  to  him  a  most  unreasonable  manner.  His  relations  to  the  men 
are  impaired.  While  still  regarding  himself  as  one  of  the  gang,  he 
develops  feelings  of  contempt  and  tendencies  to  disparage  them.  He 
still  wants  them  to  like  him,  but  he  no  longer  does  like  them,  at 
least  as  formerly,  as  equals.  His  friendliness  becomes  more  and  more 
a  cloak  for  exerting  his  authority.  He  thinks  that  a  pat  on  the  back 
and  a  smile  make  up  for  real  injuries.  At  the  same  time,  he  becomes 
hesitant  about  exerting  his  authority  or  administering  rebukes.  He 
conceals  his  disparaging  tendencies,  his  instability,  and  his  ambition 
behind  a  facade  of  irreproachability  and  imperturbable  calm.  He 
may  even  refrain  from  reasonable  and  necessary  criticism,  only  to 
burst  out  with  tangential  criticism  which  may  then  have  an  irrational 
quality.  With  the  further  impairment  of  his  relations  to  the  men  and 
his  failure  to  get  ahead  with  the  employer,  he  develops  an  increasing 
need  for  affection  which  he  seeks  from  one  woman  or  another. 

At  this  point,  our  imaginary  individual  is  hardly  distinguishable 
from  a  neurotic  personality.  An  analyst  might  well  be  able  to  ferret 
out  his  neurotic  trends;  his  striving  for  power,  his  need  for  affection, 
his  disparaging  tendencies  and  his  perfectionism.  He  would  probably 
attribute  these  trends  to  a  "basic  anxiety."  If  so,  is  it  not  clear  that  he 
has  substituted  a  convenient  abstraction  for  a  concrete  analysis  of  the 
primary  conflicts  and  the  social  situation  underlying  them?  This 
hypothetical  illustration  shows  how  necessary  it  is  to  understand  the 


502  THE  EGO  IN  PSYCHOANALYSIS 

relation  between  the  conflicts  which  appear  in  a  neurosis  and  the 
contradictions  which  inhere  in  the  practical  everyday  life  of  our 
society.  [5,  220  f.] 

That  Bartlett's  example  is  not  wholly  imaginary,  is  suggested  by 
the  findings  reported  on  p.  144  that  foremen  in  the  United  States 
tended  to  identify  themselves  with  management  in  spite  of  earlier 
allegiances  to  the  workers.  Fromm  has  noted  (particularly  in 
his  1936  study  of  authority  and  the  family  [25])  that,  the  greater 
the  contradictions  of  society  become,  the  less  chance  will  there  be 
for  an  individual  to  develop  a  strong  and  well-integrated  ego.  But 
for  Fromm  it  is  the  inherent  human  striving  for  justice  and  truth 
that  resolves  these  contradictions  and  discrepancies  arising  from 
conflicts  between  objective  conditions  and  old  norms.  He  does  not 
recognize  that  sooner  or  later  new  norms  arise  and  are  accepted 
by  the  individual  as  an  apparent  resolution  of  the  contradiction  or 
conflict. 

Neglect  of  situational  determination.  Our  last  two  criticisms  of 
psychoanalysis — its  failure  to  recognize  that  new  standards,  new 
codes  of  behavior,  or  new  norms  can  and  do  emerge  in  certain 
social  situations  and  its  failure  to  see  the  full  implications  of  social 
stratification  and  the  often  contradictory  and  conflicting  role  the 
individual  is  in  because  of  this  stratification — add  up  to  the  more 
general  criticism  that  psychoanalytic  concepts  are  unrealistic,  in- 
adequate, and  invalid,  partly  because  of  their  neglect  of  the  effect 
which  external  situations  have  on  the  individual  and  on  the  precise 
relationships  between  the  individual  and  his  external  social  world. 
In  pointing  out  the  dangers  of  the  analogy  of  the  unconscious  as 
a  storehouse,  Koffka  stresses  the  fact  that  whatever  the  contents  of 
the  unconscious  may  be  that  are  functioning  at  the  moment,  these 
are  constantly  being  transformed,  put  together  in  new  patterns, 
according  to  the  situation  presented: 

Things  do  not  simply  fall  into  those  places  into  which  they  are 
being  thrown,  they  arrange  themselves  in  coming  and  during  their 
time  of  storage  according  to  the  many  ways  in  which  they  belong 
together.  And  they  do  more;  they  influence  each  other,  form  groups 
of  various  sizes  and  kinds,  always  trying  to  meet  the  exigencies  of  the 
moment.  [38,  66,  italics  ours] 


SUMMARY  503 

While  this  criticism  is  especially  true  of  orthodox  psychoana- 
lysts, it  still  holds  for  those  who  have  tried  to  reformulate  orthodox 
concepts  with  reference  to  cultural  variables.  In  terms  of  therapy 
this  means  that  the  way  to  "solve"  social  problems  and  the  personal 
problems  of  the  neurotic  is  to  change  those  external  conditions"  that 
give  rise  to  contradictory  values  or  that  create  wide  discrepancies 
between  actual  experience  and  established  norms.  Haldane  has 
suggested  that  this  would  not  be  a  much  more  staggering  task 
than  that  of  psychoanalyzing  everybody,  for,  he  calculates,  in 
England  alone  50,000  psychoanalysts  would  be  necessary  to  keep 
even  the  people  in  that  small  country  relatively  free  from  neu- 
roses. [28,  15]  It  might  be  noted  in  passing  that  the  efforts  of 
responsible  experts  to  prevent  wars,  depressions,  and  other  major 
social  catastrophes  all  lie,  one  way  or  the  other,  in  the  direction  of 
removing  what  they  believe  are  the  conditions  that  give  rise  to 
situations  which  cause  these  catastrophes.  On  the  personal  side,  it 
is  obvious  that  a  neurosis  due  to  sexual  maladjustment  is  not  nearly 
so  likely  to  arise  in  the  case  of  a  pretty  girl  who  attracts  men  as  in 
the  case  of  a  homely  girl  whom  men  avoid;  that  a  young  man  who 
is  economically  secure  enough  either  to  marry  or  to  have  girl 
friends  is  not  nearly  so  likely  to  get  tangled  up  with  problems  of 
sex  as  is  a  young  man  similarly  motivated  biologically  but  who 
cannot  afford  the  normal  sexual  outlets  of  our  society.  Wortis 
[55]  has  pointed  out  that  many  modern  women  may  suffer  from 
the  conflict  between  their  desire  for  business  or  professional  suc- 
cess and  their  interest  in  conforming  to  orthodox  patterns  of 
femininity.8 

SUMMARY 

Psychoanalysis  at  the  present  time  has  a  tremendous  vogue. 
This  is  due  to  the  fact  that  its  method  is  dramatic  and  alluring  as 
well  as  to  the  fact  that  psychoanalysts  do  have  the  courage  to  tackle 
those  personal  problems  and  to  explain  those  social  phenomena 

8  One  does  not  have  to  imagine  instances  of  individuals  who  are  caught  in  the 
net  of  circumstances  that  constitute  their  own  relationships  to  their  social  world. 
For  example,  Professor  Edwin  G.  Boring  of  Harvard  University  has  written  an 
account  of  his  own  psychoanalysis,  of  the  personal  dilemma  he  faced  and  of  the 
emotional  entanglements  he  hoped  his  psychoanalysis  would  resolve.  [8] 


504  THE  EGO  IN  PSYCHOANALYSIS 

brought  about  in  our  present  age  of  rapid  social  change.  If  psycho- 
analysis were  concerned  only  with  individual  therapy,  we  would 
not  need  to  consider  it  in  social  psychology.  But  psychoanalysts 
do  deal  with  problems  that  fall  within  the  social  psychologist's 
domain,  and  psychoanalysis  must,  therefore,  be  closely  examined. 

For  the  social  psychologist,  Freud's  great  contribution  was  that 
he  saw  clearly  that  social  demands  of  one's  surroundings,  on  the 
one  hand,  and  instinctual  impulses,  on  the  other,  created  conflicts 
in  the  individual  and  that  no  sharp  distinction  could  therefore  be 
made  between  individual  and  social  psychology.  On  the  basis  of 
Freud's  theories,  he  and  his  followers  have  attempted  to  account 
for  a  variety  of  social  phenomena — groups  are  held  together  by 
their  libidinal  constitutions,  by  the  substitution  of  a  common  object 
for  ego-ideal;  adjustment  is  viewed  as  the  incorporation  of  parental 
images  into  the  mental  apparatus;  war  is  viewed  as  an  infantile 
regression;  revolution  is  seen  as  an  outlet  for  an  inherent  sense  of 
guilt,  revolutionary  leadership  being  motivated  by  a  paranoid 
mechanism  which  is  a  defense  against  homosexual  tendencies; 
Fascism  is  accounted  for  as  the  unconscious  indulgence  of  frus- 
trated persons,  largely  of  the  lower  middle  class,  who  substitute  a 
leader  for  the  parental  symbol  to  compensate  for  the  weakness  of 
their  own  infantilism;  anti-Semitism  is  a  projection  of  id  impulses, 
and  so  on. 

Freud's  own  formulation  of  the  ego  developing  out  of  the  un- 
organized id  and  of  the  superego  as  a  precipitate  of  an  inevitable 
Oedipus  complex  has  been  modified  by  later  analysts  who  point 
out  that  Freud  failed  to  recognize  the  facts  of  cultural  variations, 
that  the  Oedipus  complex,  for  example,  is  not  universal  but  devel- 
ops only  under  a  particular  set  of  cultural  conditions  as  Malinow- 
ski  demonstrated.  They  hold  that  Freud  posited  as  instinctually 
closed  systems  certain  psychological  mechanisms  that  can  be  more 
accurately  conceived  as  cultural  products  derived  from  a  restricted 
middle  class  capitalist  economy. 

Although  certain  of  these  analysts,  such  as  Fromm  and  Horney, 
have  made  considerable  advance  over  their  more  orthodox  col- 
leagues in  recognizing  the  general  facts  of  social  determination, 
they  have  yet  by  no  means  emancipated  themselves  from  Freudian 


SUMMARY  505 

concepts  which  are  for  them  still  the  basis  of  explanation  even 
though  their  genesis  in  the  individual  is  viewed  as  culturally  de- 
rived. Hence  criticisms  of  psychoanalysis  as  a  scientifically  valid 
and  adequate  formulation  hold  for  them  as  well. 

Scientific  procedure  demands  that  concepts  must  ultimately  be 
capable  of  naturalistic  explanation,  that  the  validity  of  theories 
must  be  demonstrable,  and  that  the  rule  of  scientific  parsimony 
is  violated  when  a  whole  host  of  concepts  is  created  without  some 
basis  in  fact.  On  these  grounds,  psychoanalysis  does  not  meet  the 
test  of  scientific  method.  This  is  reflected  in  the  frequently  bitter 
and  prolonged  disputes  that  rage  among  psychoanalysts  themselves, 
disputes  which  clear-cut  verifiable  empirical  evidence  should  be 
able  to  settle. 

Specifically,  psychoanalysis  is  open  to  three  major  criticisms  from 
the  point  of  view  of  systematic  social  psychology.  First,  it  em- 
ploys simple,  sovereign,  and  abstract  explanatory  principles  of 
motivation  which  enable  it  to  account  for  the  most  diversified  and 
contrasting  phenomena  merely  by  juggling  its  concepts  around. 
Freud  has  his  universal  Oedipus  complex  and  superego,  his  as- 
sumptions concerning  the  interrelationships  of  erotogenic  zones, 
in  addition  to  his  abstract  instincts;  Fromm  posits  the  need  to 
avoid  aloneness  derived  from  the  libidinal  structure  and  is  also 
forced  to  assume  a  universal  striving  for  justice  and  truth;  Horney 
posits  a  basic  anxiety  which  lies  behind  the  guiding  principles  of 
safety  and  satisfaction;  many  analysts  speak  of  ego-drives  as  self- 
propelled  entities. 

In  the  second  place,  psychoanalysts  fail  to  take  into  account  the 
fact  that  new  psychological  products  such  as  new  ego-attitudes 
can  and  do  emerge  under  new  conditions  or  that  frames  are  in- 
evitably developed  after  repeated  exposure  to  stimulus  situations. 
Psychoanalysis  completely  neglects,  in  this  regard,  demonstrable 
facts  from  the  psychology  of  perception,  as  well  as  demonstrable 
facts  from  genetic  psychology,  such  as  Piaget's  findings  that  moral 
codes  can  and  do  arise  spontaneously  in  children's  social  groups. 
There  is  a  continuous  dialectical  relationship  not  precisely  seen  by 
psychoanalysts  between  the  individual  and  his  social  environment, 
with  old  orientations  constantly  giving  way  to  new  and  apparently 
more  adequate  ones,  with  these  in  turn  proving  unsatisfactory  to 


506  THE  EGO  IN  PSYCHOANALYSIS 

meet  changes  brought  about  by  events,  technological  developments, 
and  the  like,  which  affect  the  structure  of  the  social  environment. 
The  social  environment,  as  well  as  the  individual's  adjustment  to 
it,  must  both  be  regarded  as  processes  where  changes  are  contin- 
ually taking  place,  where  new  norms  are  arising  on  the  stimulus 
side,  new  frames  are  emerging  on  the  subjective  side. 

Finally,  psychoanalysts — when  they  do  not  neglect  the  role  of 
culture  altogether— fail  to  recognize  the  full  implications  of  an 
individual's  placement  in  a  culture,  fail  to  see  the  consequences  of 
social  stratification,  and  thus  use  the  word  "culture"  in  an  obscure 
and  functionless  way.  A  characteristic  of  most  western  societies 
is  its  stratification  according  to  class,  status,  racial  background,  or 
some  other  group  interest.  It  is  therefore  quite  misleading  to 
assume  that  all  persons  in  a  culture  are  exposed  to  or  acquire  the 
same  standards,  values,  or  norms.  Furthermore,  since  the  values 
or  norms  of  different  groups  or  classes  within  the  society  are  fre- 
quently in  conflict  with  each  other,  and  since  the  norms  of  any  one 
group  by  no  means  always  conform  to  objective  conditions,  the 
values  or  norms  an  individual  interiorizes  and  that  become  so 
important  a  part  of  his  ego,  create  within  him  contradictory 
orientations  (frames)  which  can  and  do  give  rise  to  personal 
dilemmas  and  maladjustments  or  which  seem  unsatisfactory  to 
him  in  accounting  for  his  own  experience. 

In  brief,  psychoanalysis  by  appealing  to  simple  and  sovereign 
principles  and  by  failing  to  see  the  precise  way  in  which  the  indi- 
vidual is  related  to  and  affected  by  his  external  situation  fails  to 
account  realistically  for  man's  social  behavior. 

REFERENCES 

1.  ADLER,  A.,  The  Practice  and  Theory  of  Individual  Psychology,  trans,  by  P. 
Radin,  New  York:  Harcourt,  Brace,  1925. 

2. Problems  of  Neurosis  (P.  Mairct,  ed.),  New  York:  Cosmopolitan,  copy- 
right 1930. 

3.  ALEXANDER,  R,  Development  of  the  ego-psychology,  in  Psycho-analysis  Today: 
Its  Scope  and  Function  (S.  Lorand,  ed.),  New  York:  International  Univer- 
sities Press,  copyright  1944. 

4. Our  Age  of  Unreason,  Philadelphia:  Lippincott,  copyright  1942. 

5.  BARTLETT,  F.,  Recent  trends  in  psycho-analysis,  Science  &  Society,  1945,  9, 
214-31. 


REFERENCES  507 

6.  BARTLETT,  F.  C.,  Remembering:  A  Study  in  Experimental  and  Social  Psychol- 

ogy, Cambridge  (England) :  Univ.  Press,  copyright  1932. 

7.  BOEHM,  R,  The  history  of  the  Oedipus  complex,  Intern.  /.  Psycho-analysis, 

1931,  12,  431-51. 

8.  BORING,  E.  G.,  Was  this  analysis  a  success?,  /.  Abnorm.  &  Soc.  Psychol.t  1940, 

35,  3-16. 

9.  BYCHOWSKI,  G.,  On  relations  between  the  ego  and  the  super-ego,  Psychoanalytic 

Rev.,  1943,  30,  313-24. 

10.  FENICHEL,  O.,  Fruhe  Entwicklungstadien  der  Ichs,  Imago,  1937,  23,  243-69. 
U. Ego-disturbances  and  their  treatment,  Intern.  /.  Psycho-analysis,  1938, 

19,  416-38. 

12. The  drive  to  amass  wealth,  Psychoanalytic  Quart.,  1938,  7,  69-95. 

13. Ubcr  Trophae  und  Triumph,  Intern.  Z.  Psychoanalysis,  Imago,  1939,  24, 

258-80. 

14.  FRENKEL-BRUNSWIK,  E.,  and  R.  N.  SANFORD,  Some  personality  factors  in  anti- 

semitism,  /.  Psychol,  1945,  20,  271-91. 

15.  FREUD,  A.,  The  Ego  and  Mechanisms  of  Defence,  London:  Hogarth,  1937. 

16.  FREUD,  S.,  Formulations  regarding  the  two  principles  in  mental  functioning 

(1911),  Collected  Papers,  vol.  4,  1925. 

17.  Instincts  and  their  vicissitudes  (1915),  Collected  Papers,  vol.  4,  1925. 

18. Group  Psychology  and  the  Analysis  of  the  Ego,  London:  Intern.  Psycho- 
analytical Press,  1922. 

19. Beyond  the  Pleasure  Principle,  London:  Intern.  Psycho-analytical  Press, 

1922. 

20. Negation,  Intern,  f.  Psycho-analysis,  1925,  6,  367-71. 

21. The  Ego  and  the  Id,  London:  Hogarth,  copyright  1927. 

22.  FROMM,  E.,  Die  Entwicklung  des  Chrustusdogmas:  cine  psychoanalysischc 

Studie  zur  sozialpsychologischen  Funktion  der  Religion,  Imago,  1930,  16, 

305-73. 
23. Uber  Methodc  und  Aufgabe  einer  analytischen  Sozialpsychologie,  Z.  So- 

zialforschung,  1932,  1,  28-54. 
24. Die   psychoanalytische  Charakterologie   und   ihre   Bcdeutung   fur   die 

Sozialpsychologie,  Z.  Sozialforschung,  1932,  1,  253-77. 
25. Theoretische  Entwiirfe  uber  Autoritat  und  Familie:  Sozialpsychologischer 

Teil,  in  Studien  uber  Autoritat  und  Familie  (M.  Horkheimer,  ed.),  Paris: 

Alcan,  1936,  77-135. 

26.  Escape  from  Freedom,  New  York:  Rinehart,  copyright  1941,  reprinted 

by  permission  of  the  publishers. 

27.  GREEN,  A.  W.,  Sociological  analysis  of  Horney  and  Fromm,  Am.  f.  Sociol., 

1946,  51,  533-40. 

28.  HALDANE,  J.  B.  S.,  Psychology,  a  Marxist  Survey,  Dialectics,  1938  (?),  7,  1-15. 

29.  HORNET,  K.,  The  Neurotic  Personality  of  Our  Time,  New  York:  Norton,  1937. 
30. New  Ways  in  Psychoanalysis,  New  York:  Norton,  1939. 

31. Our  Inner  Conflicts,  New  York:  Norton,  1945. 

32.  JONES,  E.,  Mother-right  and  the  sexual  ignorance  of  savages,  Intern.  /.  Psycho- 
analysis, 1925,  6,  109-30. 

33. The  origin  and  structure  of  the  super-ego,  Intern.  /.  Psycho-analysis, 

1926,7,303-11. 

34. The  psychology  of  Quislingism,  Intern.  f.  Psycho-analysis,  1941,  22,  1-6, 


508  THE  EGO  IN  PSYCHOANALYSIS 

35.  JONES,  E.,  Evolution  and  Revolution,  Intern.  J.  Psycho-analysis,  1941,  22,  193- 

208. 

36.  KARDINER,  A.,  The  Individual  and  His  Society,  New  York:  Columbia  Univ., 

copyright  1939. 

37. The  Psychological  Frontiers  of  Society f  New  York:  Columbia  Univ., 

copyright  1945. 

38.  KOFFKA,  K.,  On  the  structure  of  the  unconscious,  in  The  Unconscious:  A  Sym- 

posium (E.  S.  Dummer,  ed.),  New  York:  Knopf,  copyright  1927,  43-68. 

39.  LOEBLOWITZ-LENNARD,  H.,  A  psychoanalytic  contribution  to  the  problem  of 

anti-semitism,  Psychometric  Rev.,  1945,  32,  359-61. 

40.  MALINOWSKI,  B.,  Sex  and  Repression  in  Savage  Society,  New  York:  Harcourt, 

Brace,  1927. 

41.  MURPHY,  G.,  L.  B.  MURPHY,  and  T.  NEWCOMB,  Experimental  Social  Psychology, 

New  York:  Harper,  copyright  1937. 

42.  MURRAY,  H.  A.,  et  aL,  Explorations  in  Personality,  New  York:  Oxford,  1938. 

43.  REICH,  W.,  The  characterological  mastery  of  the  Oedipus  complex,  Intern.  J. 

Psycho-analysis,  1931,  12,  452-67. 
44. Massenpsychologie  des  Faschismus:  Zur  Sexualoefynomie  der  politischen 

Reaction  und  zur  proletarischen  Sexualpoliti^,  Copenhagen,  1933. 
45.  REIK,  T.,  A  Psychologist  Loo  fa  at  Love,  New  York:  Farrar  &  Rinehart,  1944. 
46. Psychology  of  Sex  Relations,  New  York:  Farrar  &  Rinehart,  1945. 

47.  ROHEIM,  G.,  The  psychoanalytic  interpretation  of  culture,  Intern.  /.  Psycho- 

analysis,  1941,  22,  147-69. 

48.  SAPIR,  E.,  The  unconscious  patterning  of  behavior  in  society,  in  The  Uncon- 

scious: A  Symposium  (E.  S.  Dummer,  ed.),  New  York:  Knopf,  1927,  114-42. 

49.  Cultural  anthropology  and  psychiatry,  /.  Abnorm.  &  Soc.  PsychoL,  1932, 

27,  229-42. 

50. The  emergence  of  the  concept  of  personality  in  a  study  of  cultures, 

/.  Soc.  PsychoL,  1934,  5,  408-15. 

51.  SCHUMAN,  F.  L.,  The  Nazi  Dictatorship,  New  York:  Knopf,  1936,  2d  ed. 

52.  SEARS,  R.  R.,  Survey  of  Objective  Studies  of  Psychoanalytic  Concepts,  New 

York:  Soc.  Science  Research  Council,  1943. 

53.  SPERBER,  M.,  Schulen  und  Sekten:  sozioanalytische  Bemerkungen  zur  Situation 

der  Pathopsychologie,  Zentbl.  f.  Psychotherap.,  1932,  5,  579-93. 

54.  WAELDER,  R.,  Psychological  Aspects  of  War  and  Peace,  Geneva  Research  Centre, 

Geneva  Studies,  1939. 

55.  WORTIS,  J.,  Freudianism  and  the  psychoanalytic  tradition,  Am.  /.  Psychiatry, 

1945, 101,  814-20. 


NAME  INDEX 

(Italicized  numbers  are  reference  pages) 


Abromson,  F.,  88 

Adler,  A.,  93,  463,  506 

Alexander,  F.,  462,  466,  4*9,  471,  487, 

506 
Allport,  F.  H.,  11,  30,  57,  79,  82,  88,  90, 

281,  345 
Allport,  G.  W.,  9,  11,  18,  23,  27,  65,  70, 

88,  148,  151,  153,  381,  383.  403,  404, 

434 

Alper,  T.  G.,  128,  129,  153 
Ames,  A.,  Jr.,  16,  27 
Anderson,  H.  H.,  124,  153 
Anderson,  N.,  310,  345 
Angcll,  R.  C.,  402,  435 
Angyal,  A.,  420,  435 
Ansbacher,  H.,  73,  74,  88 
Asbury,  H.,  308,  345 
Asch,  S.  E.,  47,  53,  54,  55,  68,  69,  83, 

88,  153 
Averill,  L.  A.,  228,  229,  241,  255,  277 

Bain,  R.,  11,  177,  196 

Baldwin,  B.  T.,  277 

Baldwin,  J.  M.,   17,  27,   119,   151,  153, 

161,  162,  164,  196 
Bales,  R.  F.,  392,  393,  435 
Barker,  R.  G.,  796,  197,  279 
Bartlett,  F.,  500,  502,  506 
Bartlett,  F.  C.,  32,  41,  45,  57,  63,  64,  75, 

88,  130,  490,  507 
Bayley,  N.,  229,  277 
Bayton,  J.  A.,  138,  755 
Bean,  L.  H.,  79,  88 
Beaver,  A.  P.,  184,  187,  796 
Beebe-Center,  J.  G.,  38,  57,  58 
Beiswanger,  G.,  435 
Bellak,  L.,  83,  88 
Benary,  W.,  58 
Bennett,  A.  E.,  433,  435 
Berelson,  B.,  78,  90 


509 


Bcrl,  C.,  241 

Berman,  N.,  345 

Berne,  E.  V.  C.,  186,  796 

Bess,  D.,  384 

Bcttelheim,  B.,  399,  404,  405,  406,  407, 

408,  409,  410,  435 
Beynon,  E.  D.,  345 
Birch,  H.  G.,  77,  88,  127,  75J 
Blake,  R.,  70,  88 
Block,  H.,  68,  83,  88,  153 
Blondel,  C.,  52,  58,  280,  345 
Bios,  P.,  228,  229,  252,  253,  263,  264, 

277 

Blumer,  H.,  11,  12,  27,  280,  345 
Boas,  F.,  11 
Boehm,  F.,  468,  507 
Bogardus,  E.  S.,  11,27 
Boll,  E.  S.,  220 
Bonney,  M.  E.,  796 
Boring,  E.  G.,  58,  72,  88,  503,  507 
Bossard,  J.  H.  S.,  220 
Botsford,  R.  V.,  395 
Bradley,  P.,  277 
Braly,  K.,  71,  72,  90 
Brandt,  H.  F.,  124,  153 
Brcslaw,  B.  J.,  78,  88 
Brickner,  R.  M.,  431,  432,  434,  435 
Bridges,  K.  M.  B.,  183,  184,  796 
Bronfenbrenner,  U.,  341,  345 
Bronner,  A.  F.,  95,  775,  331,  332,  333, 

334,  346 

Bronte,  E.,  439,  459 
Brooks,  F.  D.,  277 
Broun,  H.,  82 
Brown,  A.  R.,  220 
Bruce,  A.  A.,  311 
Bruner,  J.  S.,  74,  75,  88,  129,  130,  403, 

404,  434 

Bruniwik,  E.,  73,  88 
Buck,  P.,  378,  383 


510 


NAME  INDEX 


Buhlcr,  C.,  171,  196,  249,  277 

Bunzcl,  B.,  398,  435 

Burgess,  E.  W.,  314 

Burks,  F.  W.,  230,  257,  264,  269,  278 

Butler,  S.,  455,  459 

Bychowsky,  G.,  487,  507 

Cameron,  W.  J.,  244,  257,  272,  275 
Campbell,  H.  M.,  265,  270,  271,  278 
Cantril,  H.,  18,  23,  27,  32,  47,  58,  73, 

76,  77,  79,  88,  89,  90,  91,  115,  132, 

139,    141,    151,    153,   154,   155,   282, 

345,  383 

Carlyle,  T.,  349,  383 
Centers,  R.,  73,  89,  140,  141,  142,  145, 

146,  153,  368,  383 
Chaffey,  J.,  243,  256,  260,  270,  279 
Challman,  R.  C.,  186,  196 
Chandler,  A.  R.,  58 
Chapman,  D.  W.,  32,  36,  46,  47,  53,  55, 

58,  67,  89,  121,  123,  129,  153 
Charcot,  J.  M.,  425 
Child,  I.  L.,  286,  345 
Claparede,  E.,  129,  153 
Clark,  K.  B.,  191,  193,  194,  796 
Clark,  M.  K.,  191,  193,  194,  196 
Clemmcr,  D.,  286,  345 
Cobb,  S.,  429,  430,  435 
Cofer,  C.  N.,  424,  435 
Coffin,  T.  E.,  46,  53,  58,  61,  62,  75,  89 
Cohen,  N.  E.,  38,  57,  58 
Coleridge,  E.  P.,  459 
Conklin,  E.  S.,  234,  250,  275 
Cooley,  C.  H.,  119,  151,   152,  153,  161, 

166,  176,  177,  196,  381,  383 
Cousins,  S.,  387,  388,  389,  435 
Crcssey,  P.  G.,  257,  258,  275 

Dallcnbach,  K.  M.,  59,  91 

Darwin,  C.,  159,  160,  196 

Dashiell,  F.,  281,  345 

Davis,  A.,  179,  195,  196,  249,  275,  364, 

365,  384 

Davis,  J.,  69,  59,  138,  153 
Davis,  K.,  216,  218,  220,  275,  287,  288, 

346 

Davis,  M.  H.,  797 
Dearborn,  G.  V.,  45,  55 
Dennis,  W.,  70,  55 


Dickson,  W.  J.,  366,  370,  380,  J55 
Diven,  K.,  81,  59 
Dobb,  M.,  384 
Dollard,  J.,  796,  249,  275 
Droba,  D.  D.,  11 
Dublin,  L.  I.,  398,  435 
Dummer,  E.  S.,  505 

Durkheim,  E.,  49,  52,  55.  280,  281,  337, 
346 

Ebbinghaus,  H.,  129 

Edwards,  A.  L.,  65,  76,  59,  127,  153 

Eisenberg,  P.,  401,  435 

Eisenhower,  D.  D.,  411 

Elder,  W.,  432 

Ellis,  W.  D,,  55 

Engels,  F.,  102,  775,  462,  479 

Euripides,  446,  447,  459 

Paris,  R.  E.  L.,  418,  419,  435 

Fauset,  A.  H.,  346 

Feldstein,  M.  J.,  346 

Fenichel,  O.,  471,  475,  489,  490,  507 

Ferenczi,  S.,  471 

Fernberger,  S.  W.,  59 

Festinger,  L.,  138,  754 

Fisher,  M.  S.,  177,  796 

Fitc,  M.  D.,  188,  797 

Flugel,  J.  C.,  349,  350,  384 

Folsom,  J.  K.,  27 

Frank,  J.  D.,  39,  55,  120,  122,  124,  754 

Franklin,  J.  H.,  59 

Franklin,  M.,  128,  754 

Franz,  S.  I.,  422,  423,  435 

Freeman,  W,,  432,  434,  435 

Frenkel-Brunswik,  E.,  507 

Freud,  A.,  469,  507 

Freud,  S.,  99,  281,  462,  463,  464,  465, 
466,  468,  469,  470,  471,  474,  476,  478, 
480,  481,  482,  483,  484,  485,  486,  487, 
488,  489,  490,  494,  504,  505,  507 

Fried,  E.,  59 

Fricdrich,  G.,  384 

Frisch,  R.,  87,  59 

Fromm,  E.,  462,  470,  475,  478,  479,  480, 
482,  484,  485,  486,  487,  489,  498,  499, 
502,  504,  505,  507 

Furfey,  P.  H.,  186,  796 


NAME  INDEX 


511 


Gallup,  G.  H,,  383 

Gardner,  B.  B.,  364,  365,  384 

Gardner,  G.  E.,  220,  236,  278 

Gardner,  M.  R.,  364,  365,  384 

Gaudet,  H.,  78,  90 

Gelb,  A.,  426,  43$ 

Gcsell,  A.,  172,  173,  174,  176,  796 

Gist,  N.  P.,  307,  346 

Gittler,  L.  R,  396,  397 

Golden,  C.  W.,  154 

Goldstein,  K.,  426,  427,  428,  435 

Goodenough,  F.  L.,  176,  177,  180,  196, 

797,  233,  241,  252,  254,  262,  278 
Goodman,  C.  C.,  89 
Gould,  R.,  39,  58,  122,  138,  154, 
Graham,  M.,  391 
Green,  A.  W.,  498,  499,  507 
Green,  E.  H.,  187,  797 
Grcenberg,  P.  J.,  124,  154 
Greulich,  W.  W.,  275 
Grinker,  R.  R.,  358,  384,  393,  411,  412, 

413,  414,  415,  435 
Groves,  E.  R.,  227 
Groves,  G.  H.,  227 

Haggard,  E.,  58 

Hagman,  E.  P.,  186,  797 

Halbert,  L.  A.,  307,  346 

Halbwachs,  M.,  52,  58 

Haldanc,  J.  B.  S.,  495,  503,  507 

Hall,  G.  S.,  202,  213,  214,  227 

Hallowell,  I.  A.,  276 

Halstead,  W.  C.,  426,  435 

Hankins,  D.,  227 

Hanson,  H.,  291 

Harding,  J.,  77,  89 

Hartley,  E.,  62,  89 

Hartley,  R.,  62,  89 

Hartmann,  G.  W.,  27,  65,  77,  89,  90,  91, 

385 
Healy,  W.,  95,  775,  242,  27$,  316,  331, 

332,  333,  334,  346 
Heathers,  L.  B,,  126,  154 
Hebb,  D.  O.,  433,  434,  435 
Hemingway,  E.,  440,  459 
Henri,  V.,  42,  5* 
Hcrtzman,  M.,  53,  68,  83,  88,  138,  153, 

154 


Hilgard,  E,  R.,  125,  135,  154 
Hollingworth,  H.  L.,  36,  58,  75 
Hollingworth,  L.,  223,  228,  239,  248,  250, 

260,  278 
Holt,  R.  R.,  125,  152,  754,  352,  353,  354, 

356,  384 

Honzik,  M.  P.,  797 
Hoppc,  F.,  39,  58,  120,  754 
Horkheimer,  M.,  507 
Horney,  K.,  119,  754,  470,  475,  476,  480, 

482,  484,  486,  487,  498,  499,  504,  505, 

507 
Horowitz,  E.  L.,  23,  27,  69,  70,  72,  89, 

171,  172,  797 

Horowitz,  R.  E.,  191,  192,  193,  797 
Hoskins,  R.  G.,  278 
Hughes,  E.  C.,  287,  346 
Hunt,  J.  McV.,  424,  435,  436 
Hunt,  W.  A.,  57,  58 
Huntley,  C.  W.,  128,  754 
Hurlock,  E.  B.,  180,  797,  349,  384 
Huxley,  J.,  14,  27,  106,  107,  110,  775 
Hyman,  H.  H.,  137,  138,  754,  223 

Ilg,  F.  L.,  172,  173,  196 
Isaacs,  S.,  164,  170,  797 
Israeli,  N.,  4J6 

Jack,  L.  M.,  187,  797 

James,  W.,   13,  27,  92,   118,   119,   134, 

754,  162 

Jameson,  A.  T.,  273,  275 
Jandorf,  E.  M.,  403,  404,  434 
Janet,  P.,  417 
Jannis,  J.,  278 
Janowitz,  M.,  227 
Jcbb,  R.  C.,  460 
Jennings,  H.  M.,  346 
Jcrsild,  A.  T.,  178,  188,  194,  797 
Johnson,  C.  S.,  136,  754 
Johnson,  H.,  384 
Johnson,  W.,  424 

Jones,  E.,  462,  472,  473,  494,  507 
Jones,  H.  E.,  214,  227,  230,  231,  240, 

252,  265,  266,  268,  270,  271,  278 
Jones,  M.  C.,  230,  243,  256,  260,  270, 

271,  279 
Jung,  C.  G.,  10,  27f  30,  58 


512 


NAME  INDEX 


Kardincr,  A.,  462,  480,  481,  487,  490, 

491,  499,  500,  508 
Karsten,  A.,  126,  154 
Katona,  G.,  86,  89,  90 
Katz,  D.,  11,  27,  71,  72,  76,  79,  90,  134, 

145,  154 

Katzaroff,  D.,  99,  115 
Kay,  L.  W.,  23,  27,  68,  90,  154 
Kccgan,  J.  J.,  433,  435 
Kelly,  D.  M.,  58 
Kcynes,  J.  M.,  84,  85,  90 
Kirkpatrick,  C.,  237,  278 
Kiskcr,  G,  W.,  417,  436 
Kitay,  P.  M.,  238,  27* 
Klcbanotf,  S.  G.,  436 
Klein,  G.  S.,  125,  154 
Klineberg,  O.,  13,  27,  203,  204,  227 
Klopfer,  B.,  58 
Knapp,  R.,  66,  82,  90 
Knox,  G.  W.,  417,  436 
Koch,  H.  L.,  197 
Koffka,  K.,  42,  58,  93,  99,  115,  126,  129, 

754,  171,  797,  417,  428,  429,  436,  502, 

508 

Kohlcr,  W.,  58,  171,  /97 
Komarovsky,  M.,  227,  246,  278 
Kornhauser,  A.  W.,  75,  90,   139,   140 
Kounin,  J.  S.,  196,  797,  279 
Kraines,  S.  H.,  430,  431,  436 
Kramer,  B.  M.,  65,  70,  88 
Kubie,  L.  S.,  230 
Kiilpc,  O.,  31,  32,  58 

Landesco,  J.,  297,  304,  305,  307,  311,  312, 

346 

Langer,  S.,  797 
La  Picrc,  R.  T,  280,  346 
Lasker,  B.,  11,  70,  90 
Lazarsfeld,  P.  R,  65,  78,  90,  400,  401, 

435,  436 

Lc  Bon,  G.,  280,  346 
Lcpkin,  M.,  82,  88 
Lcvine,  J.  M.,  65,  90,  127,  154 
Uvy-Bruhl,  L.,  384 
Lcwin,  K.,  171,  797,  281,  344,  345,  346, 

362,  384 

Lewis,  H.  B.,  53,  90t  128,  138,  152,  154 
Lewis,  J.  L.,  380 
Lewis,  S.,  444,  459 


Likert,  R.,  11,  71,  80,  90 

Lilienthal,  D.  E.,  362,  374 

Lincoln,  J.  F.,  371,  372,  373,  384 

Linton,  R.,  105,  776 

Lippitt,  R.,  344,  345,  346 

Lippmann,  W.,  90 

Littlepagc,  J.  D.,  384 

Loeb,  L.,  112,  776 

Locblowitz-Lennard,  H.,  508 

Long,  L.,  36,  59 

Lorand,  S.,  506 

Lowie,  R.  H.,  776 

Luchins,  A.  S.,  45,  53,  55,  59 

Lumpkin,  K.  du  P.,  776 

Lynd,  H.  M.,  215,  216,  227,  235,  236, 

246,  278 
Lynd,  R.  S.,  215,  216,  227,  235,  236,  246, 

278,  288,  346 

MacArthur,  D.,  361,  362 

Maccurdy,  J.  T.,  129,  755 

MacDonald,  L.,  367,  384 

MacFarlane,  J.  W.,  797 

Macintosh,  A.,  139,  755 

Magaret,  G.  A.,  125,  754 

Magce,  E.  S.,  227 

Mairet,  P.,  506 

Majumdar,  D.  N.,  227 

Makarenko,  A.  S.,  284,  338 

Malinowski,  B.,  11,  476,  504,  508 

Mangum,  W.  R.,  384 

Marks,  E.,  136,  137,  755,  194,  365 

Marquand,  J.  P.,  219,  227,  449,  459 

Martin,  E.  D.,  280,  346 

Martin,  J.  B.,  330,  346 

Marx,  K.,  462,  479 

Mason,  H.  L.,  436 

Matcer,  F.,  797 

Mauldin,   B.,  285,  346,   357,   358,   384t 

394,  436 
Mayo,  E.,  370 
McCarthy,  D.,  176,  797 
McCullers,  C.,  443,  459 
McDougall,  W.,  276,  421,  436 
McGarvey,  H.  R.,  36,  38,  59,  63,  90 
McGchce,  W.,  126,  755 
McGranahan,  D.  V.,  227,  397,  398 
McGregor,  D.,  90,  369,  384 
McKay,  H.  D.,  308,  330,  347 


NAME  INDEX 


513 


McNarney,  J.  T.,  359 

Mead,  G.  R,  161,  197,  452,  460 

Mead,  M.,  116,  203,  204,  205,  206,  207, 

209,  221,  235,  278 
Meenes,  M.,  72,  90 
Menefce,  S.  C.,  76,  90 
Miles,  A.,  432 
Miller,  G.  A.,  38,  59 
Miller,  H.  A.,  286,  347 
Mizanoglu,  N.,  279 
Mocde,  W.,  281,  346 
Molotov,  V.  M.,  376,  384 
Moor^  K.  C.,  163,  164,  176,  177,  797 
Moore,  T.  V.,  23 
Moreno,  J.  L.,  265,  281,  340,  341,  342, 

346 

Morgan,  B.,  435 
Morgan,  L.  H.,  102,  116 
Morlcy,  C.,  450,  460 
Mosteller,  F.,  79,  88,  91 
Mowrer,  E.  R.,  436 
Muncie,  W.,  430,  431,  436 
Murchison,  C.,  196,  197,  198,  277,  278, 

345 
Murphy,  G.,  9,  17,  27,  33,  43,  59,  65,  71, 

80,  90,  99,  776,  118,  127,  154,  155, 

281,  346,  483,  492,  495,  508 
Murphy,  L.  B.,  9,  17,  27,  99,  116,  118, 

155,  178,  183,  184,  185,  186,  797,  281, 

346,  483,  492,  495,  508 
Murray,  H.  A.,  45,  59,  89,  483,  508 
Myrdal,  G.,  365,  384 

Nelson,  E.,  12,  27 

Ncwcomb,  T.  M.,  9,  27,  73,  90,  99,  116, 

118,  155,  281,  343,  346,  385,  483,  492, 

495,  508 

Newell,  T.  E.,  384 
Newsletter,  W.  L,  342,  346 
Nutt,  A.  S.,  227 

Dates,  W,  J.,  459,  460 
Ogden,  E.,  279 
Ogden,  M.  B.,  259,  279 
O'NeiH,  E.,  440,  460 
O'Neill,  E.,  Jr.,  459,  460 
Orth,  J.,  28 

Oigood,  C.  E.,  69,  97,  755 
Oudand,  G.  E.,  346 


Page,  M.  L.,  187,  797 

Park,  R.  E.,  286,  307,  345,  347 

Parten,  M.  B.,  181,  182,  184,  797,  198 

Pavlov,  I,  P.,  51 

Penficld,  W.,  433,  434,  435 

Piaget,  J.,  161,  164,  167,  168,  169,  170, 
178,  795,  281,  335,  336,  337,  338, 
347,  486,  492,  493,  496,  505 

Postman,  L.,  38,  59,  134,  755 

Pratt,  C.  C.,  59 

Preston,  M.  G.,  138,  755 

Prcyer,  W.,  157,  159,  160,  161,  163,  164, 
166,  176,  177,  795 

Prince,  M.,  417 

Proshansky,  H.,  33,  43,  59 

Puffer,  J.,  292,  347 

Radcliffe-Brown,  A.  R.,  103,  104,  776 

Radin,  P.,  211,  212,  213,  227,  506 

Reich,  W.,  508 

Reik,  T.,  471,  487,  508 

Reuter,  E.  G.,  347 

Ribot,  T.,  425,  436 

Rioch,  M.  J.,  436 

Rivers,  W.  H.  R.,  102,  776,  227 

Roethlisberger,  F.  J.,  366,  370,  380,  385 

Rogers,  S.,  36,  39,  40,  41,  42,  59,  63,  97 

R6heim,  G.,  471,  474,  475,  508 

Roosevelt,  F.  D.,  361,  362,  380 

Rose,  G.  J.,  55 

Rosenzweig,  S.,  124,  128,  755 

Ross,  E.  A.,  280,  281,  347 

Rubinstein,  S.,  378,  379,  385 

Ruttenberg,  H.  J.,  754,  369 

Rylander,  G.,  426,  436 

Sait,  E.  M.,  125,  754 

Salusky,  A.  S.,  179,  189,  190,  795 

Sands,  G.,  165 

Sanford,  R.  N.,  507 

Sapir,  E.,  11,  309,  347,  476,  483,  496, 

497,  508 

Scheerer,  M.,  426,  427,  428,  435 
Schilder,  P.,  418,  436 
Schoenfcld,  N.,  125,  754 
Schonbar,  R.  A.,  53,  57,  59 
Schrodinger,  E.,  100,  107,  776 
Schumao,  F.  L.,  508 


514 


NAME  INDEX 


Schwab,  S.  I.,  279 

Scars,  P.  S.,  122,  123,  129,  155 

Scars,  R.  R.,  481,  483,  484,  508 

Seclcman,  V.,  64,  97,  127,  755 

Seward,  G.  H.,  238,  279 

Shackle,  G.  L.,  84,  85,  ?! 

Shakespeare,  W.,  438,  439,  442,  448,  456, 

457 
Shaw,  C.  R.,  291,  292,  293,  297,  308, 

310,   313,   315,   316,   322,   324,   325, 

326,  328,  329,  330,  333,  347 
Shaw,  F.  J.,  128,  755 
Shaw,  G.  B.,   19,  452,  453,  460 
Sheldon,  W.  H.,  10,  28 
Sherif,  M.,  17,  28,  33,  34,  48,  52,  53,  55, 

56,  59,  72,  82,  88.  91,  116,  150,  755, 

281,  347,  385 
Shinn,  M.  W.,  157,  162,  163,  164,  166, 

177,  198 

Shock,  N.  W.,  396,  436 
Shohle,  R.,  347 
Sidis,  B.,  417 
Skinner,  M.,  227 
Smith,  B.  F.,  436 
Smith,  G.  H.,  66,  97 
Sophocles,  454,  460 
Sorenson,  R.,  227 
Spcrber,  M.,  475,  477,  50* 
Spiegel,  J.  P.,  358,  384,  393,  411,  412, 

413,  414,  415,  435 
Spooncr,  A.,  128,  755 
Spranger,  E.,  10,  28,  51,  59 
Stagner,  R.,  69,  76,  79,  97,  755 
Stalin,  J.,  375,  377,  385 
Steinbeck,  J.,  438,  460 
Stern,  B.  J.,  104,  105,  776 
Stern,  W.,  65,  97,  93,  94,  776,  166,  167, 

198 

Stimpfl,  J,,  166,  790 
Stoddard,  G.  D.,  178,  795 
Stolz,  H.  R.,  243,  256,  260,  270,  279 
Stone,  L.,  356 
Stoncquist,  E.  V.,  286,  347 
StoufFcr,  S.  A.,  132,  755,  401,  436 
Strauss,  A.,  12,  28 
Strauss,  R.,  442,  460 
Strunk,  O.,  442 
Sully,  J.,  164,  165,  198 


Tanncnbaum,  P.,  347 

Thomas,  W.  L,  11,  24,  28,  273,  279 

Thompson,  H.,  172,  796 

Thrasher,  F.  M.,  290,  291,  292,  293,  294, 

295,  296,  297,  298,  299,  300,  303,  309, 

347 

Thurstonc,  L.  L.,  11 
Tiedemann,  D.,  157,  158 
Titchener,  E.  B.,  12,  13,  28t  60 
Tolman,  E.  C.,  23,  51 
Tracy,  F.,  166,  795 
Trcssclt,  M.  E.,  41,  42,  43,  52,  59 
Tryon,  C.  M.,  233,  234,  268,  279 
Tuddcnham,  R.,  229,  277 
Turnbull,  W.,  755 

Van  dc  Velde,  T.  H.,  390,  436 

Van  Gennep,  A.,  227 

Veblen,  T.,  140,  349,  350,  385 

Veeder,  B.  S.,  279 

Vettcr,  G.  B.,  79,  97 

Volkmann,  J.,  36,  39,  42,  43,  46,  47,  52, 

53,  55,  57,  58 ,  59,  67,  89,  121, 123, 129, 

75J 

von  Hofmannsthal,  R,  442,  460 
Voth,  A.,  45,  59 

Waelder,  R.,  472,  508 

Wallen,  R.  W.,  127,  128,  755 

Wallon,  H.,  95,  776,  169,  170,  179,  795 

Walsh,  W.  B.,  80,  97 

Ward,  H.  F.,  385 

Warden,  C.  J.,  109,  776 

Warren,  H.  C.,  17,  28 

Watson,  G.,  369,  385 

Watson,  W.  S.,  65,  97 

Watson,  W.  T.,  381,  385 

Watt,  H.  J.,  28 

Watts,  J.  W.,  432,  434,  435 

Weaver,  P,,  214,  227 

Weber,  E.  H.,  75,  129 

Webster,  H.,  227 

Wcdell,  C.  H.,  37,  59 

Weigl,  E.,  426,  436 

Weld,  R  P.,  23 

Welles,  O.,  81 

Wellman,  B.  L.,  178,  198 

Wcrtheimcr,  M.,  35,  44,  59 


NAME  INDEX 


515 


Wcvcr,  E.  G.,  37,  38,  40,  59 

Wharton,  E.,  258,  279 

White,  R.  K.,  345,  346 

White,  R.  W.,  230,  279 

White,  W.  A.,  417,  418,  436 

Whiting,  J.  W.  M.,  210,  221 

Whyte,  W.  R,  291,  292,  293,  294,  295, 

297,  303,  347 
Wilbur,  C.  B.,  433,  435 
Wilder,  R.  M.,  436 
Wile,  I.  S.,  235,  262,  279 
Williams,  R,  214,  221 
Williams,  R  W.,  79,  88.  91 
Williams,  R.  D.,  436 
Wilson,  N.  R,  385 
Winch,  R.  R,  279 
Wolff,  W.,  128, 155 
Wood,  C,,  64,  91 


Woodworth,  R.  S.,  31,  59,  91 
Words,  J.,  508 
Wright,  B.,  53,  54 
Wright,  H.  R,  196,  197,  279 
Wundt,  W,,  31,  163 

Yokoyama,  M.,  32 

Zachry,  C.  B.,  221,  224,  226,  227,  229, 
230,  236,  239,  240,  242,  250,  252, 
253,  257,  261,  263,  279 

Zawadski,  B.,  400,  436 

Zener,  K.  E.,  37,  38,  40,  59 

Zillig,  M.,  65,  72,  91,  127, 155 

Zimmerman,  C.,  134,  155 

Znaniecki,  R,  11,  24,  2S 

Zorbaugh,  H.  W.,  290,  291, 297,  310,  311, 
347 


SUBJECT  INDEX 


Absolute  judgment,  39 
Absolute  scales,  38  #. 
Absolute  scries,  38 
Absolute  stimuli,  method  of,  38 
Adolescence  (See  also  "Maturation") 
different  cultures  and  times,  in,  201  ff. 
duration  of  case  or  difficulty  of,  202  ff. 
problem  of,  in  sociological  and  psycho- 
logical setting,  199  ff. 
Samoa,  in,  203 /. 
social  structure,  and  the,  218 
Adolescent   culture,   252,   267    (See   also 

"Peer-culture") 

Adolescent  drives  (See  "Drives") 
Adolescent  transitions,  diverse  types  of  (See 

"Transition") 
Adolescents,    efforts    of,    to    re-establish 

themselves,  248/7. 

Adult-youth  conflict  and  changing  ego, 
239  ff.  (See  also  "Parent-youth 
conflict") 

Advertising,  psychology  of,  352 
Affectivity,  frame  of  reference  in,  34 
Age-mate  groups,  219  (See  also  "Status") 
children's  relationships  with,  186 
degree  of  influence  in  adolescence,  208  /., 

252 

effects  on,  ego  formation,  178#. 
re-formation  of  ego-attitudes,  251  ff. 
Aggression,    482    (See    also    "Personality 

traits") 

Allegiance,  conflicting,  286  (See  also 
"Bclongingncss,"  "Identifications," 
"Loyalties") 

Alonencss,  feeling  of,  239  /.,  485 /.,  505 
Altruism,  1,  152,  281   (See  also  "Identi- 
fications," "Loyalties,"  "Personality 
traits") 

Anchorages,  35  ff.,  40tf.,  87,  129,  425 
(See  also  "Frame  of  reference," 
"Reference  points") 


516 


Anchorages,    ego-involved    and    non-cgo- 
involvcd,  126 

new,  410 /. 

provided  by  values,  135 
Anchoring  point,  41  /.,   120,   122,   138 
Anchoring  stimulus,  41  /.,  63 
Anchoring  value,  63 
Anchorings,  social,  309 
Anxiety,  425  (See  also  "Basic  anxiety") 
Arapesh,  205 /. 
Arunta,  212 

Ascendance,  187/.,  343  (See  also  "Person- 
ality traits") 

Aspiration  level  (See  "Level  of  aspiration") 
Assertiveness,  105 

Attitudes,    4ff.t    93  ff.    (See   also    "Ego- 
attitudes,"  "Frame  of  reference") 

adolescent,  standardized  toward  adults, 
243 /. 

adult  gang  members,  of,  304  ff. 

affective  properties  of,  20 

change  of,  73 

characterization  of,  17-27 

class,  75 /. 

classification  of,  23 /. 

content  of,  19 

definitions  of,  17/. 

derived  from  material  conditions,  195 

discriminative  nature  of,  43 

enduring  state  of  readiness,  21  /. 

everyday  life,  in,  60-88 

experimental  facts  concerning,  29-57 

formation,  19 /. 

in-groups  and  out-groups,  toward,  297 

intensity  of,  130tf. 

memory,  and,  63  ff. 

scales,  132 /. 

selectivity  of,  34 

social,  10,  61  ff. 

social    and    economic    issues,    toward, 
140/7. 


SUBJECT  INDEX 


517 


Attitudes,  special  cases  of,  26 
status,  related  to,  21,  61 
stimuli,  range  referred  to,  23 
studies    and    methodological    considera- 
tions, 1 1  /. 

subject-object  relationship,  imply,  19 
value  judgments,  formed  from,  55 /. 

Authority,  336  f. 

Autism,  stage  of,  168 

Autokinesis,  33,  45,  49 
group  experiments  in,  52  ff. 

Autonomy,  336 

Basic  anxiety,  486,  499 

Basic  personality  structure,  3,  480  /.,  487, 

499 /. 

Behavior,  conforming,  336 
Behavior  norms,  172  ff. 
Belongingness,  5  /.,  253,  262  /.,  497  /.  (See 

also  "Identifications") 
badges  of,  261 
feelings  of,  275,  297  /. 

Castration  complex,  484 
Characterological  types,  500 
Class,  219  /.,  257  ff.,  287  /.,  310  £,  449  /., 
472,  497,  506 

attitudes,  75 /. 

consciousness,  145/.,  450 

contradictions,  284 

identifications  in  America,  139  ff. 

interests,  361 

working,   141  ff. 
Classes,  children  of  different,  195 

ego-involvements,  as,  134  ff. 
Cliques,  261  ff.,  282   (See  also  "Gangs," 
"Groups,"   "Status") 

embryonic  gangs,  as,  309 

formations  of,  girls  and  boys  compared, 
265  ff. 

girls,  among,  270  /. 

goals  of,  289 

names  of,  270 

standing  in,  249 
Clothes,  348  £ 

Codes,  296tf.,  321    (See  also  "Norms," 
"Rules") 

criminal  gang,  of,  304 

ethical,  495 /. 


Codes,  moral,  505 
Coenesthesis,  425 
Cognition,  psychology  of,  490 
Cognitive  components  in  attitude  forma- 
tion, 22 

Cognitive  reactions,  33 
Collective  behavior,  2,  51  /.,  179  ff.,  281  ff. 

(See  also  "Group") 
Collective     products,     296  ff.     (See     also 

"Group") 

Community  centrism,  201  /. 
Competition,  124,  276 

capitalist,  105 

socialist,  105,  376 
Competitive  behavior,  105/. 
Competitive  standards,  311  f. 
Concept,  3 

formation,   109#. 

hierarchic,  426 
Conceptual  level,  3,  108  #.,  113,  434  (See 

also  "Language") 
Conceptual    thought,     IWff.     (See    also 

"Language") 
Confidence,  125,  139 
Conflict  situations,  236 /. 
Conflicts,  infantile,  474 
Conforming  behavior,  29 
Conformity,  252 

established,   10 

group,  257  #. 

specific  roles,  to,  179 

tendency  to,  on  nonsecret  ballots,  132 
Consanguinity,  102 
Conscience,  95 
Contrast,  36  ff. 
Convergence,  53 /. 
Co-operation,  186/.,  276,  336  ff. 
Co-operative  behavior,  105  /.,  156,  185 
Co-operative  group  play,  181  /. 
Co-operative  relationships,  344 
Co-operative  tendencies  in  gangs,  298 
Crisis  situations,  48,  82  /.,  201 
Cultural  lag,  246 /.  (See  also  "Social  lag") 
Culture  apologists,  44 

Defense  mechanisms,  475 
Delinquency,  310 
acceptance  of  ideas  of,  331  ff. 


518 


SUBJECT  INDEX 


Delinquency,  group,  308,  313,  331 
Delinquent  girls,  273  /. 
Depersonalizadon,  4170. 
Depression,  psychological  effects  of,  216/., 

247,  399tf. 

Dialectical  materialism,  52 
Dialectical  method,  189 
Dichotomy,  experimental  and  cultural  psy- 
chology, of,  51  /. 

individual  and  society,  of,  283,  313,  334 
Domination,  3 
Drives,  96,  223 

adolescent,  275  /. 

Ego,  1*0.  (See  alto  "I,"  "Self") 
anchoring  of,  286 
boundaries  of,  152,  438 
breakdowns  of  (See  "Ego-breakdowns") 
changing,     and     adult-youth     conflict, 


components  of,  94,  98  ff.  (See  also  "At- 
titudes," "Ego-attitudes,"  "Norms," 
"Values") 

conceptual  delineation  of,  164 
constituents    derived    from    established 

society,  191  ff. 

descriptions  of,  92/.,  1490. 
development,  102 
dissociation  of,  3870. 
dissolution  of  (temporary),  391  ff. 
emergence  of,  at  human  level,  3,  108  ff., 

160  /. 
experiences  from  stimulation  of  group, 

188  /. 

formation,  94,  113,  156-195 
effects  of  some  social  factors  on,  178  ff. 
language  development,  and,  176$. 
some  effects  of  age-mate  groups  on, 


instability  of,  in  adolescence,  222  ff. 
localization  of,  171 
masculinity-feminity,    incorporation    of 

norms  of,  232  ff. 
psychoanalysis,  in,  461  ff. 
re-formation  of,  in  adolescence,  199  ff. 
reification  of,  412 
word,  much  abused,  1,  92 
Ego  aspirations,  276,  355 


Ego-attitudes,  4/.,  920,  1560.,  212, 
386  ff.  (See  also  "Attitudes,"  "Ego," 
"Norms") 

age-mate  groups  on,  effects  of,  251  ff. 
constellation  of,  92,  405 
contradictory,  238 
disintegration  of,  431 
group  norms,  from,  321  ff. 
group  situations,  structured  or  modified 

in,  334 
reference     and     membership     groups, 

formed  in  relation  to,  496 
Ego-breakdowns,  95/.,  107,  153,  3860, 
455      (See      also      "Personality," 
"Schizophrenia") 
abnormal    and    pathological    cases,    in, 

4170. 
collapse  of  established  norms,  because 

of,  396  ff. 

extreme  deprivation,  under,  393  ff. 
surgical  insult  to  frontal  lobes  on,  ef- 
fect of,  431  ff. 
war-created    situations    on,    effect    of, 

412  ff. 

Ego  distances,  102 
Ego-disturbance,  139,  417,  449 
Ego  drives,  4,  101  ff.,  274,  505 
Ego-enhancement,  153,  350,  354 
Ego-frustration,  153,  156 
Ego-gratification,    105,    153,    156,    350, 

445 /. 

Ego  ideal,  468£,  504 
Ego-inflation,  452 
Ego-insecurity,  139 
Ego-involvements   (See  also  "Ego,"  "I," 

"Self") 

characterization   of,   general,   92-115 
co-operative  work,  in,  152 
degree  of,  134 

determined  by  situation,  3570. 
experimental  literature  on,  1470. 
experiments  on,  117-153 
intensity  of  attitudes,  and,  1300. 
laboratory  tasks,  in,  1180. 
lack  of,  389 /. 
learning,  as  set  for,  126  ff. 
status    and    class    identifications,    and, 

1340. 
thresholds  of,  4 


SUBJECT  INDEX 


519 


Ego  level,  39,  120,  126 

Ego-misplacement,  153 

Ego  need,  101  ff.t  152 

Ego  reference,  100  /.,  417,  420  ff.,  426 

Ego  relationships,  changing  nature  of,  95  /. 

Ego  shifts,  200 

Ego-striving,  3  ff.t  277,  312,  434 

individual   differences   in,   98,   106 /. 

not  instinctive,  101-115 

opposite  goals,  towards,  105  /. 
Ego-values,  122,  252,  283 /. 
Egocentricity,  136,  173 

coefficients  of,  178,  224 
Egoccntrism,  stage  of,  168 
Egoistic,  151 /. 
Emergence,  neglect  of,  on  social  level,  494 

new  qualities,  of,  51  /. 
Emergent,  conceptual  level  as,  108/. 

human  culture  as,  109/. 

neglect  of,  qualities,  487  ff. 
Encephalitis,  430 
Erotic  trends,  467 
Eroticism,  anal,  472 
Expectations,  84  #. 

Extraversion,   187    (See  also  "Personality 
traits") 

Failure,  group,  123 

tasks,  in  assigned,  124 
Fantasy,  167,  249,  284,  335 
Fascism,  7,  473,  482 
Fixations,  personal,  150 
Foremen,  study  of,  142/. 
Forgetting,     selective,     127/.     (See    also 

"Memory") 

Frame  of  reference,  30  ff.,  40  ff.,  129,  425 
(See    also    "Anchorages,"    "Refer- 
ence points,'*  "Reference  scale") 
arTectivity,  in,  34 
artists',  30 /. 

breakdown  of  usual,  41 8  ff. 
economic  works,  in  some  recent,  83-87 
ego-involving,  120  ff. 
established,  62 
expectations,  and,  86 /. 
experimental  inculcation  of,  55  ff. 
formation  of,  48  ff. 
in  group  situations,  51  ff. 
in  unstructured  situations,  48  ff. 


Frame  of  reference,  formation  of,  problem 
of,  490 /. 

individual,  55 

judgment,  effect  upon,  121  ff. 

laboratory  situations,  in,  50  ff. 

memory,  in,  63 /. 

new,   410 /. 

perceptual  and  judgmental  activity,  in, 
34-43 

personal,  68  /. 

prisoner's  life,  of,  406 /. 

structured   and   unstructured   situations, 
in  relation  to,  43  #. 

unstable  and  conflicting,  286 

use  of  term,  34 /. 
Fraternities  and  sororities,  259/. 
Frontal  lobes,  surgical  insult  to,  431  ff. 

Gangs,  282  (See  also  "Age-mate  groups," 
"Cliques,"  "Groups,"  "Status") 

adult,  304 

areas  of,  292 

control  of  members  of,  302 

developing  from  play  groups,  293/. 

disintegration  of,  303  /. 

function  of,  291  ff. 

goals  of,  289 

impact  of  society  on,  3Wff. 

names  of,  296 

number  of  members  of,  295 /. 
Goals,  8,  176 

cliques  or  gangs,  of,  289 

ego-striving,  of,   105  /. 
Gorki  Colony,  284,  338 /. 
Grcgariousness,  333 /.  (See  also  "Person- 
ality traits") 

Group    (See   also    "Collective    behavior," 
"Collective  products,"  "Loyalties") 

allegiances,  98 /. 

behavior,  differential,  281,  299 

formations  in  everyday  life,  269  ff. 

"group  decision,"  362 /. 

identifications,  99,  287  ff.,  314  ff. 

influence  earlier  for  maturing  girls,  256  /. 

interactions,  development  of,  334#. 

loyalty,  299 

members  influenced  by  group  norms, 
,  317  tf. 


520 


SUBJECT  INDEX 


Group,  norms,  124  /.,  493 

pressure,  125,  296,  298 

relationships,  reciprocal,  102 

situation,  formation  of  frame  of  refer- 
ence in,  51  ff. 

slogans,  297 

solidarity,  269,  297 

Groups  (See  also  "Age-mate  groups,1* 
"Cliques,"  "Gangs,"  "Membership 
groups,"  "Reference  groups,"  "Sta- 
tus") 

artificiality  of  sharp  lines  between,  309  /. 

democratic  and  autocratic,   344 /. 

deviant   behavior,   prolonging,   329  /. 

escapist  solutions,  with,  284 

generality  of,  307  ff. 

isolated,  286,  321 

psychological     properties     of,     266  ff., 
317  ff. 

spontaneous,  307  #.,  316  ff. 

structural  properties  of,  288#. 

values  of,  284,  327 
Guilt,  sense  of,  494 

Hero  worship,  260 

Heteronomy,  336 

Higher  mental  processes,  31 

Homosexual    tendencies    of    revolutionary 

leaders,  495 

Hostility,  unconscious,   473 
Human  nature,  1,  156,  283,  382  /.,  437, 

480,  485 /. 
libidinal  basis  of,  494 

'I"  (Sec  also  "Ego,"  "Self") 
concept  of,  113 

self-  or  social  consciousness,  as,  166 
Id,  464  ff. 

inheritance  of,  471 

Identifications,  7,  115,  184,  290,  443 /. 
(See  also  "Ego-involvements,"  "Loy- 
alties") 

conflicting,  264,  288 
group,    287  #,    307,    358  #,    374/7., 

392  /.,  415  /.,  497 /. 
individuals,  with,  260  /, 
Jn-groups,  with,  194 


Identifications,  movie  stars,  with,  351 

objects,  with,  467 

parents,  with,  467 

racial,  191  ff. 

Soviet  workers,  of,   377  ff. 
Idol  worship,  3 
Idols,  250 /. 
Imagining,  32 /. 
Impulse  to  work,  498  ff. 
Inadequacy,  feelings  of,  331  /. 
Income  group,  139  ff. 
Individual,   behavior,   direction  of,   330 

group  norms  and  identifications,  as  i 

flucnced  by,  312$. 

Individual  differences,  6/.,  10,  115,  15 
274  /.,  284,  379,  386 

ego-striving,  in,  98,  106/. 

gang  members,  of,  291  , 

status  and  leadership,  and,  182,  294 
Individualism,  276 /. 

goal  of,  477 

Individualistic  behavior,  106 
Individualistic  methods,  337 
Individualistic  standards,  31  If. 
Individuality,  biological  basis  of,  112/. 

breaking  down  of,  408 /. 
Infantile  regression,  504 
Infantile  sexuality,  200,  473,  484 
Inferiority,  feeling  of,  93,  463 
In-groups,     345     (See     also     "Cliques," 
"Gangs,"  "Membership  groups") 

attitudes  toward,  297 

identifications  of,  194 

members  of,  270 

solidarity  of,  340 
Inhibition,  432 
Initiation  ceremonies,  202  ff. 
Ink  blots,  45 

Insecurity,  feelings  of,  216,  288 
Instincts,  96 

death  and  destruction,  483 

erotogenic,  471 
Intensity  scale,  132 
Internal  factors,  32  /.,  45  ff. 
Interpersonal      relationships,      reciprocal, 
108  ff.    (See  also   "Relationships") 
Interviewers,   working  and  middle  class, 
145 


SUBJECT  INDEX 


521 


Job  satisfaction,  368  ff. 
Judgment,  68 

affective,  57 

distribution  of,  40  /. 

everyday  life,  in,  61  ff. 

expressive  behavior,  of,  128 

frame  of  reference  in,  34  ff. 

ill-defined  situations,  of,  69 

lability  of,  47 

level  of  aspiration,  and,  126 

physical  characteristics,  of,   136/. 

psychology  of,  489 

relativity  of,  37  ff. 

social  and  non-social  stimuli,  of,  52 

social,  73 

stereotypes  on,  effects  of,  72 

value  dimensions,  and,  63 
judgmental  activities,  29 

referential  nature  of,  36  ff. 

Kinship,  constellations,  102 
relationships,  102  #. 
systems,  103 /. 

Laboratory,  everyday  life,  and,  12  ff.,  16 

situations,   social   factors   in,   50#. 
Language,   109  ff.,   164  /.,   181    (See  also 
"Concept,"  "Conceptual  level") 

development,  163 

ego  formation,  and,  176  ff. 
Learning,  22,  30 

classical  laws  of,  129 

ego-involvements   as   "set"   for,    126  ff. 

ego-  versus  non-ego-involved  situations, 

in,  130 

Leadership,    democratic    and    autocratic, 
344 /. 

development,  182 

individual  differences  in,  182,  294 /. 
Level  of  aspiration,  39,  67/.,  120$. 

experiments  on,  129 

frame  of  reference,  and,  36,  121,  123  /. 

generality  of,  126 

judgment,  and,  121,  126 

lowering  of,  403  /. 

Negro  college  men,  of,  138/. 
Level  of  performance,  124 
Libidinal  constitution  of  groups,  470  /. 
Libido, 


Lobectomy,  113,  434 
Lobotomy,  113,  432  /. 
Loyalties  (See  also  "Allegiances,"  "Belong- 
ingness,"  "Identifications") 

conflict  of,  380 

groups,  to,  269  /.,  299 

national,  mixed,  367  /. 

reciprocal,  320 
Love,  439  ff.,  471 

Marginal  man,  7,  238,  286  /. 

Marginal  position,  365,  486 

Marginality,  5,  222,  248,  251,  264,  419 

Martyrs,   356  /. 

Marxism,  127 

Masculinity-feminity   (See  "M-F  test") 

Masculinity  complex,  238 

Material  surroundings,  179  ff. 

Maturation,  3 

adolescence,  early  and  late  in,  229  ff. 
Member-character,  34,  37,  44 
Membership  groups,  7,  114/.,  223,  274/7., 
496  (See  also  "Cliques,"  "Gangs," 
"Status") 

adolescent     ego-attitudes,     effects     on, 


psychological  functions  of,  309 

status  in,  97,  99 
Memory,  experiments  on,  32  /. 

finished   and   unfinished   tasks,  of,   128 

frame  of  reference,  and,  63  /. 
Mental  chemistry,  31 
M-F  test,  233 
Middle    class    tradition   and    ideology    in 

America,  140 
Moieties,  104 

Moral  development,  161,  335  ff. 
Motion  pictures,  350  /. 
Motivation,  20,  51,  87/.,  113/.,  274  tf. 

Names,   ego-involvement  with,   442  /. 

psychology  of,  352  ff. 

transition  to  adulthood,  in,  202 
Narcissism,  99 
Needs,  96,  165,  168 

narcissistic,  472 
Neurotics,  bourgeois,  480 
Nonconformity     in     adolescent     groups, 
262  ff. 


522 


SUBJECT  INDEX 


Norms,   11,  97  ff.t  114/.,  130  (See  also 

"Standards  of  conduct,"  "Values") 
adolescent,  251  ff. 
adolescent  cliques,  of,  271  /. 
body  proportions  and  growth,  of,  227  ff. 
breakdown  of,  under  stress,  394  #. 
collapse  of,  effect  on  ego,  396  ff. 
contradictory,  201,  239 
established  social  order,  of,  179,  191  ff. 
evaluation  of,  14 /. 

group,  257,  281  #.,  289  #.,  304,  320 
highly  differentiated  societies,  of,  203 
imposed,  335 
individual,  54 
majority    group    accepted    by    minority 

group,  of,  364  ff. 
masculinity-feminity,     of,     200,     225, 

232  If. 
social,  68 

society  and  gangs,  of,  326 /. 
spontaneous  emergence  of,  in  age-mate 

activities,  336 
superstructure  of,  47  /. 
survival  of,  47/.,  236  /.,  246 /. 

Object-cathcxis,  467 

Oedipus  complex,  247,  463,  467  #.,  476, 

478,  483  #.,493,  504 
Organic  disturbances,   425  ff. 
Out-groups,  345 
attitudes  toward,  297 

Panic,  81  /. 
Paranoid  defense,  495 
Parent-youth  conflict,  217,  224  (See  also 
"Adult-youth   conflict  and  chang- 
ing ego") 

gang  membership,  and,  329 

sociological  analysis  of,  287/. 
Parental  images,  471 
Paresis,  general,  430 
Participation  in  groups,  470 /. 

progressive  development  of,  1790. 
Peer-culture,   253    (See  also   "Adolescent 

culture") 

Peers,  229  (See  alto  "Age-mate  groups") 
Perception,  frame  of  reference  in.  34  ff. 

new  relationships,  of,  492 

psychology  of,  490  f. 


Perception,  selectivity  of,  30  ff. 

social,  129,  136 

value  and  need,  effects  on,  73 /. 
Perceptual  objects,  131 
Perceptual  reactions,  33  ff. 
Perceptual    stage    in    attitude    formation 

19/.,  43 

Pcrsonalistic  psychology,  93 
Personality,  7,  95,  235,  328,  403  /.,  410, 
432    (See  also   "Individual   differ- 
ences") 

brain  lesions,  effects  on,  429 /. 

characteristics,  97 
body  changes  in  adolescence,  and,  231 

culture,  and,  480 

differences,  45,  56 

dissolution  of,  425 

factors,  400 

frame  of  reference  in,  34 

social  determination  of,  477 

split  and  multiple,  422  ff. 

transformation  of,  418 
Personality    traits,    114,    178,    187,    295, 
299  /.,  328  (See  also  "Aggression," 
"Traits") 

Phantasy  (See  "Fantasy") 
Physiological  changes  in  adolescence,  225  /. 

changing  self,  and,  226  ff. 
Physiological  imbalance,  434 
Pleasure  principle,  168,  465 
Points     of     reference     (See     "Reference 

points") 

Political  interest,  intensity  of,  134 
Prejudice,  102,  194,  284 

characterized,  26 
Primacy  effect,  52,  56 
Primary    groups,    297,    342     (See    also 
"Cliques,"      "Gangs,"      "Groups," 
"Membership  groups") 
Profit  motive,  374 
Progressive  education,   339  ff. 
Protective  techniques,  45 
Prostitution,  258,  387  ff. 
Psychoanalysis,  appeal  of,  461 

interpretation  of  ego  in,  412,  464  ff. 

science,  as,  481 

social  psychology,  relationship  to,  462 
Psychodrama,  341 ' 


SUBJECT  INDEX 


523 


Psychological  deficits  of  psychodcs,  424 
Psychological  properties  generated  in  group 

situations,  280  #. 

"Psychological  weaning,"  223,  241,  287 
Puberty,   199,  224  ff. 

rites,  213 
Public  opinion,  measurement,  11 

poll  results,  71,  77,  79  ff.,  134,  139  ff. 

Quislingism,   473 

Radicalism-conservatism,  145  /. 
Reality  principle,  465 
Recall,  128 /.  (See  also  "Memory") 
Recapitulation  theory,  161 
Recidivism,  329 /. 

Reference  frame  (See  "Frame  of  Refer- 
ence") 

Reference  groups,  7,  114/.,  137/.,  217, 
223,  274  ff.,  326,  496  (See  also 
"Status") 

adolescent  ego-attitudes,  effect  on,  252  ff. 
gangsters,  of,  305  tf. 
status  in,  97,  99 
Reference  idols,  223 

Reference  points,  41  /.,  49,  62,  73,  93,  97, 
122,  137  (See  also  "Anchorages," 
"Anchoring  point") 
ascription  of  status,  for,  105 
Reference  scale,  49tf.,  52,  57,  131  ff.,  136 
Regression,  404,  409,  415 
Relationships,   reciprocal,   6,   108^.,   170, 

179tf.,  334  ff. 

Representations,  collective,  49 
Repression,  467,  469 
Responsibility,  inner,  337 
Retention,  128 /. 

Role,  134  ff.,  168,  289  #.,  402,  452 
Rorschach  test,  45,  61  /.   (See  also  "Ink 

blots,"  "Protective  techniques") 
Rules  (See  also  "Norms,"  "Values") 
game,  of  the,  322,  335 
observance  of,  179 

practice  of  and  consciousness  of,  335  ff. 
Rumors,  66/.,  82 

Sadomasochistic  impulses,  478 
Samoa,  106,  203  /.,  276 


Schizophrenia,  250,  418#.,  424,  433  (See 
also  "Ego-breakdowns"  "Person- 
ality") 

Security,  feeling  of,  299 
Self,  151  /.,  164,  420 /.  (See  also  "Ego," 
"Ego-breakdowns,"     "Ego-involve- 
ments," "I,"  "Personality,"  "Schizo- 
phrenia") 

changing,  224  ff, 

-competition,  122 

conception  of,  124 

consciousness  of,  118/.,  161,  169 

-destruction  (See  "Suicide") 

-discovery,  174 

-esteem,  119 /.,  121,  152,  350 

-evaluations,  127 /. 

experience  of,  169 

-interest,  131 

-judgments,  122 

-respect,  400 

social,  119,  134 

somatic,  162 

-words,  166 

Selfish  interests  and  needs,  151  /. 
Senility,  430 /. 

Set,  26,  31#,  1260,,  426 1 
Situation   (See  also  "Stimulus  situation") 

closed,  338 

ego-involving,  117/.,  125 

medium  and  high  structure,  of,  53 

stress,  399  ff. 
Situation-set,  31 
Situational  approach,  343 
Situational  determination,  7,  178/.,  275 

ego-involvements,  of,  357  ff. 

leadership,  of,  295 

neglect  of,  502  /. 

parent-child  relationships,  of,  247 
Situational  factors,  300 
Slogans,  82  /.,  297 
Social  factors  in  laboratory,  50  ff. 
Social  lag,  461   (See  also  "Cultural  lag") 
Social  movements,  83,  86,  282,  312 
Social  norms  (See  "Norms") 
Social  perception  (See  "Perception") 
Social  pressure,  122  ff. 
Social  stratification,  264  (See  also  "Class") 
Sociality,  186/.,  334  (See  also  "Personality 
traits") 


524 


SUBJECT  INDEX 


Society,  impact  of,  310 /?. 
Sociogram,  265  ff.,  340  /. 
Sociology,  authoritarian,  337 

spiritualistic,  52 
Sociomctry,  341 /. 
Solidarity,  262,  296  If.,  320 
Soviet  Union,  127,  138,  189,  284,  338 

adolescents  in,  218 

personality  characteristics  in,  235 /. 

roie  of  worker  in,  375  /. 
Stakhanov  movement,  376 /. 
Standards  o£  conduct,  421  /.,  456 /.  (See 

also  "Norms") 
Status,  6,  252  ff.,  375  ff.,  448  ff.,  497 

adult,  200  ff. 

changes  in,  200  ff. 

conception  of,  419 

contradictions  in,  211,  217,  287 

criteria  of,  137/. 

differences,  500 

dimension  of,  137 

gang,  in,  294  ff. 

genesis  of,  137/. 

group,  in,  363  ff. 

hierarchy  of,  364 /. 

identifications,  134  ff. 

loss  of,  400 /. 

marginal,  420  (Set  also  "Marginal  man," 
"Marginal  position,"  "Marginality") 

meaning  of,  137/. 

measurement  of,  343 

men  and  women,  of,  233  ff. 

obligations  of,  295 

reference  and  membership  groups,  in, 
97  ff. 

reference  points  of,  105 

relationships,  168 
effect  of  unemployment  on,  401  /. 

satisfaction  with,  137/. 

standards  and  society,  311 

treatment,  and,  452  ff. 
Stereotype,  26,  69  ff.,  80 
Stimulus  scale,  42 
Stimulus  situation   (See  also  "Situation") 

ambiguous,  62 

gradations  of  structuration  in,  45/7. 

prestige  labeling,  and,  77 

structured,  43/.,  68,  137 

unstructured,  33  /.,  44  ff.,  68 


Stratification  (See  "Class,"  ''Social  strati- 
fication," "Status") 
Striving,  justice  and  truth,  for,  485  /.,  505 

masochistic-sadistic,  485 
Subjectivity,  335 
Sublimation,  474,  484 /. 
Submission,  3,  105,  482  (See  alto  "Per- 
sonality traits") 
Subnormal  individuals,  107 
Substitutive  activities,  284,  331 /. 
Success,   Assigned  tasks,  in,  124 

definition  of,  individual's,  138 

group,  123 

Suggestion,  46,  61  /.,  82  /. 
Suicide,  250,  356  /.,  398  /.,  408,  454 /. 
Superego,  467  ff.,  505 
Superiority,  longing  for,  93,  463 
Superstructure,  44,  205,  489 
Supra-local  qualities,  37 
Sympathetic  behavior,  183/f. 
Systems,  closed,  340,  478 

Technology,  advancement  of,  368 

changes  in,  104,  287,  493 

impact  of,  on  less  developed  peoples,  201 

level  of,  179 
Temperament,  106,  404 
Testimony,  psychology  of,  65 
Thematic  apperception  test,  45 
Traits,   309,   343    (See  also  "Personality 
traits") 

divergence  of  sexes  in,  233 /. 

infantile,  337 

stability  of,  403 /. 
Transition,  218 

adolescence,  in,  200-205,  213  (See  also 
"Adolescence") 

country  in,  244 

rites,  212 /. 

U-shaped  curve,  132 
Unconscious,  collective,  474,  478 

instincts,  488 

storehouse,  as,  502   • 
Underworld,  305 /.,  312 
Unemployment,  399  ff. 
Unstructured    situations    (See    "Stimulus 
situation")