Skip to main content

Full text of "Hewa phonemes: a tentative statement"

See other formats


2. Outline of Phonemes 

2 . 1 Chart of phonemes 

2.1.1 Work chart of contolds* 



p 

[p ph pi 


bl 


t 
[t th t^ ] 


[U 




P 

[p b] 




[s zl 




X 


■ 




n 

1 

[I T f] 







w y 

2.1.2 Work chart of vocolds 

t u 

[t t] [u u] 

e o 

C A 3 

a 



^ Notice that there Is no /h/ phoneme, even though the people and their 
language are called [*heiia]. This is a Duna word meaning 'sun*. (The Hewas 
all live at lower altitudes than the Dunas.) All significant government 
contact has come through the Lake Kopiago post in the Duna language area. 



< . Ir-tfii"pretation ;f /ixbiva." f.Tit Seginents and Sequences 

; . 1 status of vocoid segments 

!.:.! Word-flrjal vocoids- -lost or non-existent? 



The question of whether some words were losing their final voweis or 
whether there were in fact closed syllables was a major concern throughout 
the early analysis. After much reading, especially Hooper (1976) and 
Lehiste (1970). a pattern emerged from the data. Native speakers also 
recorded comparative data which was analysed with the aid of an 
oscilloscope and "Its printed record. A pattern became clear showing Hewa at 
present still has word final vowels, although phonetically some words 
regularly end in a consonant, even in isolation. 

Basically, the data showed that /;/ and /t,/ were being dropped 
word-finally following /« n I s/. Since /i/ and /c/ are two of the shortest 
vocoids in intrinsic duration ( Swadesh 1947:139; Lehiste and Peterson 
1961:275; Lehiste 1970:18; dt Chene 1979:21), any process that shortens or 
absorbs any of their duration would tend to cause their complete 
disappearance. lax allophones Ti u] already cannot occur word-finally in 
Hewa. The vowel .'a/' is already a lax vocoid and is easily dropped 
word-finally. Section 3.1.2 attests to this loss of features as a general 
pattern word-finally in Hewa. (Hooper (1976:235-40) discusses vowel 
deletion as a historical process of losing features until finally becoming 
unnecessary altogether.) 

Two additional items serve as supporting evidence. One is that Hewa 
as a stress language would tend toward such a pattern of deletion (Hooper 
1976:227, 236). Regular word stress on the first syllable aids such a 
word- final deletion tendency. Being far from one stress , but needing to 
appear weak in relation to the stress on the next vowel spoken, the 
word-final vowel devoices, or becomes aspiration on the preceding 
consonant, or deletes altogether. The other item is that some of the most 
common vocabulary appeared to be most consistently af fected--words such as 
[ 'Bcan ] 'boy' , I 'en an ] 'girl ' , [ ' warn] 'wild animal ' , [ * 1 1 1 i o« ] 'cassowary* , 
['Kip] 'pig', l'cI: 'he', ['Lnicll 'sickness' , [ '■ Is] 'long, tall' , [ 'y is] 
'smoke*. Hooper (1376:104) states, "Phonological change works its way 
through the lexicon, moving from item to item.... the frequency of use of a 
lexical item plays a part, i.e., more frequent items undergo change before 
l^ss frequent items." She indicates that an erdeletion word-finally has 
occurred in Spanish in this way. 

Part of the early confusion was caused by /[/ and /c/ very rarely 
being lost at the end of the past tense verbal suffixes /-ac/ [-bc] 'far 
past, realis' and /-yc/ [~yc] 'recent past, realis'. It wasn't until the 
pattern of vocoid sequences began to be clarified in the verbs, and 
limitations seen on sequence length there (section 3.2.4), that I realized 
each vowel or vowel sequence in the verb carried meaning, even the final 
vowel. Therefore, it could not be dropped, as occurred in other parts of 
speech being composed of single morphemes as words. 

The consonants mentioned above ( /m n ! s/') also pattern to facilitate 
this deletion word-f inally . As cont iuuauts they more readily are capable 
of absorbing a very short vowel's voicing or duration into their own. This 



54 Workpapers In Papua New Guinea Languages 



is seen to be a universal tendency by Hooper (1976:206-29) as she details 
her Universal Strength Hierarchy for consonants . 

This loss of word-final vowels occurs with all vowels in the normal 
flow of speech, especially when the preceding consonant is one of those 
mentioned above, or when the next word begins with a voweI--especially one 
which is phonetically close to it in its formation in the mouth, or when 
the last consonant of one word is phonetically similar to the consonant 
beginning the next word. 

1. /'ana '30u/ ['an *pau] 'I have none.' 

I neg. 

2. /'clc a'pu/ E'cI a'3u] 'Where is he?' 

he where 

3 . / *napa ' atapa ' nasi / [ ' nap^ ' atapa ' was] 'Let 's all go ! ' 

we.pl all hort .go 

4 . / 'napa 'no 'dIo '■otoyai ' nc le/ 

we, pi place there stay .cont .past that 

['napa 'nolo 'moto'yai 'nelcj 'That is where we were staying.' 

5. /'ho pi 'pile 'wi A'pe/ [ 'mob 'pile * wi a ' pc ] 

man that name what 'What is that man's name?' 

As vowel loss is predictable, all words shall be interpreted as having 
a final vowel . 

3.1.2 Voiceless vocoids and aspirated stops 

Voiceless vocoids occur in the word-final position particularly when 
the preceding consonant is a stop. This is due to the normal stress 
pattern, as mentioned above. In addition, as pitch drops lower and lower 
following the highest pitch on the stressed syllable, it compounds the 
problem of speaking or hearing final vowels. All the vowels have been 
recorded as voiceless word-finally. They will be represented as the voiced 
vowels with which they vary, but never contrast. 

/ '«as I / [ *«asl ] - C * mas] 'must go' 

/wipe/ [ ' wi pC] - [ ' wi pc] - [w i p] 'domestic pig' 

/'wase/ ['wasE] - ['wase] 'Let's do it!' 

/'tiuBA/ i'tiuwA] - E'tium] 'place name' 

/' wapa/ [ 'aapQ] - [ '«apa] ' forehead ' 

/' wawa/ [ 'wauwO] - [ ' wauwo] ' intend to eat ' 

/'topo/ ['topO] - ['topo] 'Put it (there)!' 

/'topu/ ['tapUl - [*tapu] 'clouds' 

Aspiration of voiceless stops [C^] in the word-final position occurs 
infrequently, but varies with silent vocoids or voiced vocoids. The 
aspiration will be represented by the voiced vowel with which it varies. 

/'wolapi/ ['wolaph] ^ ['wolapl] - ['wolapi] 'all right' 
/'lati/ ['ioth] - [-lotl] " ['lati] 'arm' 



Hewa 55 



/•titc/ ['tith] - ['tite] - E'titc] 'this' 
/•Ris€tc/ ['wiscth] - ['wisctc] 'cold' 

/•noke/ E'nokh] - ['nokei 'bird' 

All voiceless vocoids and aspirated word-final stops will be 
represented phonetically hereafter as voiceless vocoids, but phonemically 
as voiced vowels. 

3.2 Status of vocoid sequences 

Although eight vowels are being proposed in this analysis, only seven 
co-occur regularly in sequences. The vowel /a/ has not been found to occur 
in sequences other than [Ia] and [ua]. 

Seven vocoids can theoretically combine into forty-two pairs (not 
including geminates). Thirty-four of those pairs have been recorded in 
Kewa speech and are listed in Figure 1 in descending order of the number of 

actual sequences occurring . 

Following Vocoid 







-i 


-u 


-0 


-a 


-e 


-c 


-0 


p 


i- 


— 


iu 


io 


ia 


ie 


ic 


io 


r 


u- 


ui 


— 


uo 


ua 


ue 


uc 




e 


o- 


oi 


ou 


— 


CO 


oe 


0€ 






a- 


oi 


au 


ao 


-- 








V 


e- 


ei 


eu 


60 


ea 


— 


ee 


eo 


o 


c- 


c\ 


cu 


CO 


CO 




— 


CO 


c 


D- 


oi 


ou 


00 


OQ 






— 



Figure 1. Vocoid Pairs Heard in Hewa 

3.2.1 Sequences of mid and low vocoids 

No more than two vocoids articulated with mid and low tongue height 
are ever found together at any one time within a word. Thirteen of the 
twenty possible combinations from Figure 1 have been recorded. Each pair 
takes the timing of two syllables. Primary or secondary stress may apply 
to either member of a pair. 

'male name* 

'affirmative response ' 

'vomit * (noun) 

'the Raggiana Bird-of -Paradise' 

'must regularly do' 

' therefore ' 

'female name' 

•have not done' 

'Wake (him) ! ' 

' Come 1 ' 

'already assembled' 

'knows how to carve' 

'from the centre, in the middle' 



[oa] 


/ * lol oapa/ 


[Molo^apa] 


[oe] 


/o'e/ 


[o'e] 


toe] 


/'koe/ 


['koc] 


[ao] 


/' yao/ 


[ ' yao] 


[eo] 


/' Baseo/ 


[ * MQseo] 


[eo] 


/'keleoBc/ 


[*kcle2a»G] 


[ec] 


/' lecBo/ 


[* Iec«a] 


[eo] 


/•peo/ 


[■peo] 


[co] 


/ ' tawc I co/ 


[ • ta"wclco] 


[ea] 


/sc'a/ 


[sc'o] 


[CO] 


/uc ' anco lopi / 


[no ' onc^o lopI ] 


[do] 


/'«oo/ 


[•■00] 


[oa] 


/'poapo/ 


['poapQ] 



56 Workpapers in Papua New Guinea Languages 

These pairs are interpreted to be two syllables phonemically. 

3.2.2 Sequences beginning with high vocoids 

Other vocoids which combine regularly in pairs are those where the 
first is articulated with high tongue height and the second may be any 
vocold. Eleven of the twelve possibilities from Figure 1 have been 
recorded. The only two sequences with [a] found to date are of this type. 



[lu] 


/•tlU«A/ 


E'tiUBA] 


'place name' 


[to] 


/'■€kio/ 


['■ckio] 


'customarily si 


[ial 


/t i ' aacne/ 


[t i ' aacne J 


' slowly' 


[Icl 


/• 1 ieopa/ 


[» I ie^'opa] 


'male name' 


CiG] 


/■sic/ 


['sic] 


' (I) don't know' 


[lol 


/'ti3/ 


C^tio] 


'ours [dual ) ' 


[ui] 


/' su i»o/ 


[ * suiaa] 


' female name ' 


[uo] 


/■a' na luo/ 


[■A* na luo] 


'customarily speaks' 


Cua] 


/• luo/ 


[' lua] 


'wife' 


[ue] 


/* DDuenoHc/ 


[ ' * op'e^naaC] 


•dragonfly' 


Cue] 


/'■aluenla/ 


[ 'aal u^cn ia] 


'six (adj. ) • 


[1a] 


/'OpItlAPA/ 


[ *DpLtiAPA] 


'male name* 


[ua] 


/*nukuApe/ 


['nuk"Ape] 


'morning' 



Stress occasionally falls on the second vocoid of such pairs, showing 
the Independence of each of the vocoids, as for example, 

/kopt'uaa/ [kopl'uaa] 'male name' 
/tn I 'cpl/ [ttl I 'cpl ] 'afternoon' 
/u'asi/ [u'osl] 'owner'. 

In addition, most words of four syllables or longer have a secondary 
stress which falls on the third syllable when counting the primary stress 
as the first syllable. Often this secondary stress splits pairs of 
vocolda, as narked above. On this basis, these vocoid pairs are also 
interpreted as two vowels being peaks of two syllables. 

3.2.3 Sequences ending with high vocoids 

All ten pairs of a mid or low vocoid followed by a high vocoid have 
been recorded (Figure 1). Those written phonetically as [yv] generally 
take the timing of only one syllable, but those written [VV] generally take 
the timing of two syllables. {The key difference between ['ao"] 'descend' 
and [a3*u] 'swim', or [*ya"] 'dog' vs. [ya'u] 'sun', is not contrasting 
tialng, but contrasting stress.) 

[o*] /*axol/ ['agoi] 'sweet potato' 

[ow] /'aopou/ ['aopou] 'will wrap/bind' 

[ai ] /'palkoo/ [ *pa)koa] 'tapioca' 

[a"] /■tonuBOune/ [* tanu*ao"ne] 'Pull!' 

[e»l /'Icl/ [Mel] 'Jungle vines' 

[eu] /'aaneu/ [*aaneu] 'will get' 

Cc>] /'aelwD/ ['ac'so] 'intend to see' 

Ccul /'acu/ ['acu] 'will have' 

[3 » 1 / • to i ae/ [ ' to lac ] ' strong ' 

[o"! /'ouno/ ['auno] 'dip food in sauce' 



57 



As these are basically the reverse patterns of the pairs discussed 
immediately above (3.1.2), and since no two-vocoid pairs having the same 
stress pattern contrast their timing as one syllable vs. the timing as two 
syllables (e.g. [o'j vs. [a.i]), these pairs ending in high vocoids shall 
be regarded as the phonetic manifestations of the two vowels in sequence. 

3.2.4 Sequences of three or more members 

There seems to be a limitation on the duration of vowel sequences, 
causing them to be pronounced as closely as possible to the timing of two 
r.id or low vowels in sequence as two syllables, e.g. [e-o]* (See 4.3.3.) 

When three or more vocoids are heard together, at least one is a high 
vowel. Its duration is always brief and is always tied to the preceding 
segment. When it follows a consonant it causes the consonant to sound 
labialized or palatalized, e.g. /'kueala/ ['k^e'^alo] 'male name' (3.2.3). 
When the high vowel follows another vowel, it is pronounced as the 
off -glide, e.g. /'tiliau/ ['tili^a"] 'bat ' and /'mouanu/ [* «o"^anu] 'deep, 
well-anchored (roots)'. Secondary stress also breaks the sequence as a 
consistent feature. 

When a sequence of three or more vocoids is potentially formed by 
morphemes Joining within words, a semivowel is inserted to break up the 
string or to replace a high vocoid. Since all Hewa verb roots end in a 
vowel or vowel sequence, and since six vocoids are each a verbal suffix 
morpheme, this insertion occurs regularly. 

/'■ia/ 'is going' [ ' m i a ] < {•i-a} 

go continuous 

/'■iu/ 'will go' ['Miu] < {ai-u} 

go future 

/ ' mi you/' 'will be going' [ ' m iya" ] < (mi -o -u } 

go cont fut 

/'■jyc/ 'recently went ' I'miyc] < {ini-i -c } 

go nr.pst realis 

/* wi Hj, ' intend to go ' [ *■ i w j ] < (ni i -u -3 } 

go fut irrealis 

/'miyanj.' 'Intend to be going' I'miyo^uj] < !mi-o -u -o) 

go cont fut irrealis 

Examples where geminates are created across morpheme boundaries are given 
in Section S . : . 

Since the neaning is carried heavily In the vowels of the adjoining 

morphemes, Hewa speakers separate poter.tlal squeezes and possible loss of a 

vowel. The insertion of /; / fits a ;:niversal pattern in handling such 
cases (Hooper, 242) . 



58 Workpapers in Papua New Guinea Languages 



3.3 Status of contoid clusters 

No unambival'snt contoid coasters are to be found within the onset or 
coda slots of Hewa syllables. Therefore, all phonetic clusters heard in 
such positions are to be reinterpreted. (Clusters across morpheme 
boundaries are discussed below in Sec. 6.2). 

3.3.1 Word-Initial clusters 

Contoid clusters are found only when the first syllable is unstressed, 
and consist of a stop or fricative plus lateral. Such contoids will be 
separated by re-inserting [l]/ which appears to have weakened in the 
unstressed syllable to the point of being lost. (Hooper (1976:235,39) 
states, "Stress languages which usually have vowel reduction processes, 
insert or delete the minimal vowel.") 

/'pile/ L*pLle] - ['pile] - ['pcle] ' mouth ' 

{The Sepik Hill Stock root for mouth/ teeth is [p i -b 1 ] . ) 
/p i 1 e ' ka/ [pi e ' ka] 'teeth' (mouth's appendages) 
/p i J i ' pa i apa/ [pi i ' pa ' apM 'male name ' 
/t i ' I i / [ • tf I ] 'the Blue Wren Warbler' 

3.3.2 Labialized stops, fricatives and nasal 

All stops and fricatives (except /x/) and the nasal /■/ can be found 
in /Cu/ syllables when not followed immediately by another vowel. Since 
pairs of vowels occur in all environments, these [C'V] will become /CuV/. 

/' Dpuenawc/ [ 'op^enamc ] 'dragonfly' 

/' nukuApe/ [ ' nuk^Ape] 'morning' 

/'tutuapo/ ['tutWapAJ 'from that far (place)' 

/'kui/ ['k"i] 'the Huli people' 

/'pue/ ['p^c] 'outer wrapping' 

/'■ucBc/ [ '»"c«g] 'wove' 

4. Description of Phonemes 

4.1.1 Description of consonantal phonemes 

/p t k p s x/ Apart from /t/ the obstruents vary freely with their voiced 

allophones in initial and intervocalic positions.^ The stops also vary 
freely with their aspirated allophones in both of those positions. 



2 Some secondary evidence for [b] not being a full phoneme was noted when 
words from Tok Pisln which begin with /b/ were generally pronounced by Hewa 
speakers with /■/ or /k/ or /p/. 

C'»csLkLs] biskit 'biscuit, cracker' 

['■LRAtaq] binatang 'insect' 

[ ' kusbo * ] bosboi ' government representative ' 

[ ' po* } boi 'boy, male' 

[ ' p iS ] bis ' beads ' 

and/or pis 'tinned fish' 



5e««a 



59 



/' puko/ 
/' napa/ 
/ ' pu ( u / 


[•puka] - t'buka] 
[ 'napa] - [ 'naba] 
[ 'pul u ] - [ 'p^u lu] 


'male name' 
'we (plural) ' 

'hunting bow' 


/* tapu/ 

/'otcte/ 

/•tIto/ 


[ 'tapu] 

[•otete] 

I'thito] - ['thitho] - 


'clouds' 
'work' (n.) 
['tito] 'here' 


/*ka«u/ 

/'nikat/ 

/'kDkopa/ 


[ 'k*>a«u] - C'ko»u3 
['nlkai ] - [ 'nigai ] 
[ 'kokopQ] - ['kogop^ ] 


'frog' 

•eye' 

•nose' 


/'pcne/ 
/'■opi/ 
/a'pu/ 


[ 'pcne] 

[•■opi ] - [■■obi] 

[a'pu] - [A'bu] 


'you (dual) • 
'married male' 
'where?' 


/' aaslo/ 
/' sunc/ 
/'si sale/ 


[ 'masi o] - [ 'muz io] 
[ 'sunc] 
[ 'sLsale] 


'must go' 
'Snap it! ' 
•ferns' 


/ ' xo i pa/ 
/' axa/ 
/' saxD/ 


[ ' xo *pa] 
[*axa] 
[ "saxo] 


' gourd ' 

'mountain range' 
' bone spoon ' 



The velar consonants are backed further when preceded or followed by a 
low or back vowel. 



/' nokc, [ ' nok^ ] - [ ' no9c] 'bird' 

/'kona/ ['kono] - ['^tona] 'banana' 

/'pakAma/ ['bakAao] - ['bakAao] 'heel of foot' 

/'axol/ ['oxoM - ['ago*] 'sweet potato' 

/• 3X3 1 one/ [ ' Dxolone] - [ 'oicolane] 'new' 

The stops may be unreleased word finally when the final vowel is 

dropped. 



/'wolapi/ ['wo lap I] - ['wolap^] 
/ 'wi pG/ [ 'wipe] - [ 'wip"" ] 



/' iwitc/ 
/* tiata/ 



[• iwLtc] - [• iWLt^ ] 
['tiota] - I'tiot^ ] 



'It's all right' 
'domesticated pig' 

'down lower on mountain* 
'immediate vicinity* 



/'papakuku/ [^papa^kukL] - [ » papa^kuk'' ] 'area name' 

It is difficult to find many pairs of words where /x/ and /k/ phonemes 
contrast clearly, yet there are a number of other reasons, including the 
pattern pressure and my own "feel" of the language, for positing /x/ as a 
full phoneme with limited distribution. Some of my reasons are: 

-. [k k^ g k 9 X g y] as allophones of one phoneme is to overload the 
s i ng 1 e phoneme /k / . 

2. To group [k k^ q \f. g] as /k/ and [x g ^] as /x/ follows the clear 
pattern of the allophones of /p/ vs. /p/ and /t/ vs. /s/. 



60 Workpapers in Papua New Guinea Languages 



3. Cochran (1968) proposed multiple phonemes /k x g/ for the western 
dialect of Hewa. 

4. Hepburn (1980) proposed multiple phonemes /k "^ h/ for Sanio, the 
only other language within the Sanio Family to have been analysed. 

5. Dye and Dye (1965) proposed /k g *> h/ for Bahinemo (Gahom) , the 
next language eastward from Sanio in the Sepik Hill Stock to have 
been analysed. 

6. Bruce (1979:36) proposed /k g h/ for the far eastern end of the 
Sepik Hill Stock. He also proposes a proto-Sepik Hill sound system 
including /*k *g *h **?/ (1979:494). 

On the basis of all these factors supporting an analysis of there 
being more than one back consonant phoneme, I am positing /x/ as a 
full phoneme. 

/« n/ The voiced nasals occur in all positions in a word. 

/'■cone/ CaeanC] 'unmarried male' 

/'pisa«a/ ['pLsaMQ] 'sugar cane' 

/' one/ [ * and 'me' 

/*nok€/ [ 'nokG] 'bird' 

/I/ [f] (infrequently) and [T] vary freely with [1] in intervocalic 
position. 

/'ale/ ['ale] ~ [afe] 'shoulder' 

/o'lapcni/ [a'Tcpcnl] - [a'fapen] 'upper arm' 

/ti ' I 1/ EtL' I i ] - [ 'tf i ] 'Blue Wren Warbler* 

/tA'nalu/ [lA'nalu] * [tA'noTu] 'Speak! ' 

/'niliac/ ['nLliaaj - ['nLliaa] 'two days away from today' 

/' lua/ [ ' lua] 'wife' 

fm y/ The voiced semi-vowels occur word Initial and medial in non-peak 
slots of syllables, [w] tends to follow a back vowel ( 69i»s of word 
medial examples in the data); [y] tends to follow a front vowel (80% 
of word medial examples in the data) ; this is characteristic of Sepik 
River basin languages (Pike 1964:130; Hepburn 1980:7). 

/*wolapi/ [•polapl] 'all right, O.K.* 

/'wiwa/ ['wiwa] 'bark rope' 

/'towai/ ['topa»] 'Hang (it) up!' 

/'ylllwa/ ['yLllwa] 'male name' 

/a«e'ya/ [a«e'ya] 'fruit bat' 



4.1.2 Free variation between full phonemes 

Some words have been heard where there appears to be free variation 
between pairs of full phonemes in certain words, not as a general pattern 



Hewa 



across all words. These variaticDns are generally intervocalic, but 
occasionally word initial. The predominant form is listed first in each 

example , 



61 



/p/ ' >/ 



/'pokole/ - /pokole/ 'grass' 

/'vaiapa/ - /yaiapa/ 'male name' 

/t/ - Is/ 

/'takupa,' - "sakupa/ 'white haired possum' 

/ ' yakasonc/ - /' yak a tone/ 'area name ' 

(This place is at the edge of the language area, towards the Duna 
language. Some people there are bilingual with Duna. The government 
from Kopiago uses the /s/ form of the name. Duna has no /t/ vs. /s/ 
distinction. (Cochrane 1966) ) 

Part of the reason for the /l/-/s/ variation may be the influence from 
Duna, where the three major areas of that language correspond to a split 
partially along a /t/, /s/, /ts/ variation. The Lake Kopiago dialect, 
which borders Hewa, uses /s/. Many loanwords from Tok Pisin with /t/ in 
them are pronounced by Hewa speakers with /s/ or varying /t/ - /s/ 
because those words entered via Duna speakers. 

/I/ - /y/ 

The choice of /!/ or /y/ seems to be a matter of personal preference. 

'yati/ ~ /'lati/ 'male rope belt' 

/ ' iiDp i 1 i / - . ' nop i y i / 'dark- fur red possum ( pha 1 anger ) ' 

/k/ - /x/ 

/'kaipa/ - /'xaipo/ 'breadfruit' 

/'poxo/ - /'pake/ 'sugar cane pith' 

4.2 Description of vowel phonemes 

All vowels are found word initial, medial, and final. 

\\.\ is an allophone of [i] and occurs when followed by a nasal or when the 
vowel of the following CV syllable is a high vowel. 

luj is an allophone of [u] and occurs word medially in the environment of 

/p/. 

"iwcli/ ['iwcli] 'sleeping mat' 
.''niapo/ ■ 'niapa] 'age mate of the same sex' 

/ ' i I i pa/ [ ■ L 1 ipa J ' bone* 

/'sinu/ ['smL] "^ -' --7 i into the house)!' 

/ ' i ncpt:/ [ * LHcpe] 'weapon ' 



62 



Workpapers in Papua New Guinea Languages 



/'enapa/ ['enapa] 'heart' 
/'mewo/ ['mew3j 'intend to do' 

/'maneu/ ['maneuj 'will get, hold' 

/ • Gp imc/ L ' cb LinG ] 'already announced ' 

/pc' tcnc/ [pc ■ tenC] 'young' 

/«c 'ancDiopi/ [mG^anc^oIopi] 'already assembled ' 

/'Ana/ ['Anal 'do like that, is like that' 

/'tAiDCla/ ['tAmcljj 'roof' 

/*»AXAli/ ['mAXAli] 'to hollow out, bore a hole' 

/'alualu/ [*alu^alu] 'fruit dove' 

/'atapa/ ['atapa] 'all' 

/'yao/ C*yao] 'Raggiana Bird-of-Paradise * 

/'ule/ ['ule] 'head covering' 

/'kusiau/ ['kusia"] 'Ecclectus Parrot' 

/'pupete/ ['pupGtc] - ['pupctc] 'pain' 

/'wupc/ ['wupc] 'yesterday, tomorrow' 

/'opa/ ['opaj 'hole/den in a tree' 

/•oto'to/ [mato'to] 'must gather' 

/'oxolonc/ [3xol3nC] 'new' 

/■yDkole/ [ 'yokale] 'testicle' 

/' l3p3/ ['13P3] 'theirs (plural)' 

4.3 Suprasegmental Items 

4.3.1 Pitch 

Pitch is the most important factor of the suprasegmental features in 
recognising the stressed syllable, although intensity and length are 
interrelated. Only one syllable per word carries the highest pitch and 
normally it is the first syllable. The final syllable has a low, fading 
pitch. All other syllables have an intermediate pitch regardless of 
whether they follow the high pitch or occasionally precede it. Absolute 
pitch is not important. What is significant is the relationship of syllable 
pitch within one word. 

'SS / 'wi .pc/ 'pig' 

/•yoT^/ 'Raggiana Bird-of-Paradise' 

/*a7Ti/ 'shoulder' 

' mar r i ed woman ' 



•SSS / ' aio - I u . a/ 



/' i . I i .pa/ 'bone' 



'SSSS /'ku.Uu.tG.tc/ 'spider' 



/' pi • pa . I a . le/ 'a hawk species' 



63 



S ' SS u . ' tu who/ 
S'SSS" /si .^ la.ku.m^/ 



SS'SS /ya.pQ.'si.nn/ 



'elbow, knee' 
' i^zards ' 



-T 



"U 



SS'SSSS /nu . p i . ' sa .u . na . no / 'the Honeyeater tird' 

SSS 'S /ma . to . po . ' 1 a/ 'must dispose of (something) ' 

Contrastive pitch patterns occur only when location of the stressed 
syllable contrasts within a single string of phonetic segments. These 
patterns are overlaid on the next examples below (Sec. 4.3.2) to avoid 
repetition of data and to show the close relationship between the 
suprasegmental features . 

4.3.2 Intensity 

Within a word there is one syllable which carries slightly greater 
intensity than the rest. In the data under analysis more than 88X of the 
words have that syllable first. The other syllables within the word are 
heard with an intermediate intensity, generally decreasing as the word 
progresses. The final syllable has the least intensity, often being 
decreased to the point that distinctive features of the final vowel are 
lost. (See Sec. 3.1 above.) This is particularly true of final syllables of 
the shape 

Im-l-i i 

in- i-€ i 

1 1 - ! -A I 

Is-l-al 

where the vowel has disappeared almost completely in the speech of a large 
percentage of the Hewa speakers. 

Even with the high predictability of the first syllable having greater 
intensity, some words contrast only on the basis of which syllable carries 
both the greater intensity and highest pitch. This combination of greater 
intensity and highest pitch creates the stressed syllable and will be 
symbolised by / * / phonemically . 



/ ' matano/ ['ma. la. no] 

/ma* tone / [«a . ' to .no] 



'bean' 

'must listen' 



/' tcic/ 
/tc' \c 

/ ' a^o ! c / 
/a ' ^^I c / 




'that distant (thing)' 
'Scrape it! ' 

' grave ' 
'Where? ' 



64 



Workpapers in Papua New Guinea Languages 



/ 'yau/ 
/ya' u/ 

/'sic/ 
/si 'c/ 




[si .'c] 



'dog' 
'sun' 

' (I) don't know' 
'Shoot! ' 



Of those words where stress is not on the first syllable, it is more 
often on the second than on another syllable. Examples of this as well as 
stress on the third or fourth syllables are given in section 4.3.1. 

A secondary Intensity {^) may be heard in words of four or more 
syllables, and falls two syllables behind the one with primary intensity 
(M- Being predictable, this secondary intensity will not be written 
phonemically. 



/ 'pipal ale/ 
/ke* ol UApA/ 
/'pi picme/ 

4.3.3 Length 



[^pipa^lale] 
[ke^alu^APA] 

[1 pLpi ^CUC] 



' a hawk spec i es ' 

'a male name' 

' sharp , pointed , tapered ' 



Length has been recorded with vowels but not with consonants. 
Lengthened vowels occur only in the first syllable and only when that 
syllable is stressed. Such lengthened vowels vary with regular length 
vowels in the same word and will not be represented phonemically. Only two 
words have ever been heard consistently with length and not the variation 
mentioned. They are both personal names and will be written without length 
for the present. 



/'tlpo/ 
/'kapi/ 



L'ti.pa] 
[•ko.pi] 



*a male name' 
'a male name' 



Non-contrastive Features 



5 . 1 Onomotopoea 



The words heard to date are related to animals and the natural 
elements. These include the following: 

/*p3polo/ 'bubbles, boiling action, gurgling stream' 
/'nopupu/ 'a strong wind (associated with rain)' 
/*u/ 'high-pitched howl of a dog' 

(Hewa dogs don't bark.) 
/'eya/ 'a bush fowl which makes this cry'. 

/*mc\a/ 'cicadas, the time when they screech (approx. 6:30 pm) 

/'wolawolo/ 'a bird which makes this cry (Black-headed Pitta)* 



Hewa 



65 



5 . 2 Reduplication 

Examples include single morphemes which have no meaning associated 
with only the portion which is repeated. Others that do have meaning will 
be broken down for clarity. 



/ ' a i u a I u . ' 
/'walcHioIe/ 
f ' lopc I ope/ 
/ ' noma/ 
/' 3p i Dp i / 
/" 3 i i 3 1 i / 
/'kukueme/ 
/* itutuapa/ 



a fruit dove* 

pleasant ' 

a draft or gentle air movement' 

dance leader ' ( /bo/ 'beat drum' ) 

small portions or pieces' (/opi/ 'part') 

on top ' ( /d 1 i / ' on ' ) 

chatted' (/kue/ 'words') 

from very far down the mountain' (/tua/ 'far' 



5,3. Nasalisation 

Nasalisation has been heard consistently only on the words which are 
the last three examples in section 5 . 1 above, all animal sounds . It is 
interpreted as non-phonemic, 

6 , Unsolved Issues 

6.1 Consonant clusters in Duna names 

Less than thirty words with consonant clusters of apparent Duna origin 
have been recorded. These clusters occur word medially, and consist of a 
nasal plus a stop of the same point of articulation. In Duna, they are 
prenasalised stops, but are pronounced by Hewa speakers as consonant 
clusters with a syllable boundary between the two members. 

Of these, twenty-one are personal names. These individuals are of all 
age groups and were born or raised on the edges of the Hewa language area 
bordering the Duna language area. They have at least one Duna grandparent 
and/or a parent bilingual with Duna. 



[mb] 



[ ' kcmba ] 
[ ' amb i I a J ] 



Lfid ] [ ' t Ltid i QKo] 

[ ' anduku ] 



Lag J 



[ ' paw i oqgo 3 

[ ' I opangi 1 



male name' 
female name' 

male name' 
female name' 

male name' 
female name* 



Of the remaining words, one is a food which comes only from altitudes 
higher than the Hewa language area, i.e., the Duna and Ipili language 
areas, ['oogal ' pandanus nuts'. 

Another plant name is [kampe] 'reed'. 

One word of unknown origin is a beetle name, ['ko»batio] 'rhinocerous 

beetle' . 



66 



Workpapers In Papua New Guinea Languages 



The last words are related to seeing: 



[ ' Loga' ] - [ * niko ' 
[tq'kikal ] 



' eye ' 

'vision, eyesight ' 



Hewa speakers appear to have no problem pronouncing these names for 
two reasons. One is the adaptation of making voiced stops voiceless or 
accepting the voiced form as the variant allophone normally found between 
voiced segments (see 4.1.1). The other reason is that nasals word-finally 
(perhaps It is morpheme-f inally) regularly lose the following vowel, 
especially in the flow of speech (see 3.1.2 and 4.3.2). That process 
regularly brings consonants together across word boundaries or morpheme 
jTinctions (6.2). 

/'wa«G tili'oBc/ ['wam ti.li'o»] 'the wild cassowary' 

wild. animal cassowary 



/ * waac ' sua/ 
wild. animal bandicoot 



[ * warn ' sua ] 



'the wild bandicoot' 



/'eaanc pe ' tene/ 
girl young 

/ ' one tD*n3/ 

me give! 



[ ' e«on pc 'ten] 



[ 'an to' no] 



'young girl ' 



'Give it to me! 



As it appears that Hewa speakers sense a break coming between such 
adjoining consonants, it will be assumed that the borrowed names from Duna 
have been Hewa-cised and can honestly be represented with Hewa phonemes. 



/■p/ from [«b] 
/' kcwpa/ 
/' QKpi I ai / 
/'koBpat io/ 
/'koMpe/ 

/nt/ from [na] 
/'tintlawa/ 
/* antuku/ 

/nk/ from [Hg] 
/*pa»i anko/ 
/* lapank i / 
/' anka/ 
/in'klkai/ 



[kcaba] 
['aabi laH 
[koabat io ] 
C 'koabe] 



'male name' 
' female name' 
'rhinocerous beetle' 
' reed' 



['tLudiaaa] 'male name' 
['onduku] 'female name' 



[ ' pawiaqgo] 
[ ' lapaqgi ] 
[ 'aqga] 
[iQ'kikai ] 



'male name' 
' female name' 
'pandanus nuts' 
'vision, eyesight' 



6.2 Consonant clusters across morpheme boundaries 

Hewa has very few words which are composed of two or more morphemes. 
These appear to be limited to the pronouns, where a final vowel changes; 
the verbs, where all roots end in a vowel; and the locatives. Only in the 
locatives do consonants appear to cluster across morpheme boundaries, as 
some 'location' -morphemes end in a nasal and all 'direction' -morphemes 
begin with consonants, as shown below. 



67 



hoc Morpheme Dir Morpheme 

OIB- -SU 

upstream towards 

Lii- -tut^apQ 

downstream far- from 

won- - p i 3 1 C 

across there nearby 

3- -pulapQ 

higher from 

i- -wtti 

lower at 

Any ' location' -morpheme of the first column must join with any one of 
the 'direction' -morphemes in the second column to form a locative word. 
All combinations are possible. Of all the morphemes indicated the first 
three 'direction' -morphemes have been found to stand free. There is no 
known association or borrowing from Duna here. 

/ C? ) / L'amsu] * in the direction of upstream' 
/ (?) / [ 'wontutwapQ] 'from far across (the valley)' 

The patterns of pitch and intensity function as if each locative is 
one word. The time given to pronounce the word ['amsu] is the same as 
other two-syllable words. To leave the analysis with this as the 
conclusion would appear to require positing two additional syllable types, 
viz. VC and CVC , both being found only as first syllables of locatives or 
in borrowed Duna names. This does not seem justified. The cluster appears 
to be sufficient clue to the Hewa speaker and hearer to mentally divide the 
word between the two members of the cluster on the basis of morphemes. 

If the word-final vowel loss is regarded as morpheme-final instead, 
then the earlier solution would apply here. 

An alternative would be to insert the minimal vowel /g/ behind the 
nasal in the ' location ' -morpheme creating */'amcsu/. However, when later 
written, this may be pronounced *['a.mc. su] after the manner of /ome'ya/ 
[a.ne.'ya] 'fruitbat*, as a three-syllable word. This would have to be 
watched when a practical orthography is taught. 

Some examples are set out here : 

/'amcsu/ ['amsu] 'in the direction of upstream' 

/' imcpi::)lc/ [LmpiolCj 'nearby on the downstream side' 

/' woncpu I apa/ ; ' wjnpu I opQ] 'from across on the mountain' 

/'DtuluapQ/ ['DtutwopQ] 'fror. far up the mountain' 

/'iwiti/ ['jALtl] 'low on the mountainside' 

The better solution, I feel , is to regard the 'direction ' -morphemes as 
unstressed clitics which are not bound, but purely are unstressed words 



es Workpapers in Papua New Guinea Languages 



causing a locative phrase to phonetically sound like on; word. The 
word-final vowel loss pattern would thus still be app: cable. The examples 
Immediately above would be represented like this phonemically and 
phonetically: 

/ ' amc su/ [ ' amsu 3 

/ ' i»c 3 i Die/ [ ' Lap iolc] 

/'monc pulopa/ ['wonpuIapQ] 

/'3 tutuapo/ ['otut^apQ] 

/■ i witi/ [* iWLt!] 

7. Distribution of Syllables and Phonemes 

7.1 Syllable patterns 

Hewa syllable structure may be summarized by the formula: 

S = (C) V 

Two types of syllables may be generated from this formula and bo^h are 
found in use. These are CV and V. Initial, medial and final positions 
within a word are open to either syllable type. Words may be from one to 
eight syllables in length. Some examples are given below. 

One-Syllable Words - 2 of the 2 possible patterns occur. 

V. /'3/ 'skin, leaf 

CV, /'wa/ 'handheld drum* 

Two-Syllable Words - 4 of the 4 possible patterns occur. 

V.V /'do/ 'string bag' 

V.CV /'aki/ 'chin' 

CV.V /nio/ 'eye area' 

CV . CV / ' t3 I c / ' tongue , flame ' 

Three Syllable Words - 7 of the 8 possible patterns occur (there are no 
V.V.V patterns) . 

V.V.CV /' aita/ 'father' 

V.CV.V /'axoi/ 'sweet potato' 

V.CV.CV /'alclo/ 'hunger {n. ) ' 

CV.V.V /"koai/ 'the Friar Bird' 

CV.V.CV /'nicpa/ 'hair, fur, feathers' 

CV.CV.V /'lipio/ 'theirs ( dl . ) ' 



Hewa 69 



CV.CV.CV /'kamcni/ 'mosquito' 

Four-Syllable Words - 15 of the 16 possible patterns occur (CV.V.V.V does 
not occur) . The incidence of this word type is much higher when there are 2 
or more CV syllables in the pattern. 

V.V.V.V. /'aiai/ 'fight, argument' 

V.V.CV.CV /'aipcse/ 'trunk of tree' 

V.CV.V.CV /'iniclc/ 'sickness' 

V. CV.CV.CV /o'loDcnc/ 'upper arm' 

CV.V.V.CV /'lieopa/ 'male name' 

CV . V . CV . CV / ■ m i cs i mc / ' deaci ' 

CV.CV.V.V /'toneau/ ' H^nd it (to me)!' 

CV. CV.CV.CV /yapo'sina/ 'lizards' 

Five Syllable Words - 16 of the 32 possible patterns occur. Those patterns 
which do occur must have a minimum of two CV syllables. If only two such 
syllables are present, they cannot be adjacent to each other. Examples are 
nearly all personal names or verb forms. 

Six-or-More-Syllable Words - Very few of the vast number of possible 
patterns occur. As in the five-syllable words above, there must be 2 CV 
syllables in a six-syllable word, and more in seven and eight-syllable 
words, but they cannot all be adjacent to each other. Many verb forms are 
six syllables, but the few longer verb roots create seven- and 
eight-syllable words. The longest word recorded is /mc * anco I opi owe/ 
'(people) already were gathered'. 

7.2 Co-occurrence of consonants with vowels in syllables 

All the consonants may fill the onset slot and all the vowels may fill 
the peak slot of either syllable type. Of the eight possible V syllables 
and the 88 possible CV patterns, only eight CV syllables do not occur. 
These involve combinations with /x// and /e/ and /a/. Front vowels and 
high vowels are not found following .'x/ (*/xi/# */xe/, */xc./, */xu/» 
*/xa/). The other syllables not found are */sa/, */yA/ and */ye/. 

7.3 Vowel Clusters 

When three vowels come together they consist of arrangements of seven 
of the vowels, but not /a/. At least one vowel in a string of three must be 
a high vowel, and it may be in any of the three positions. Two vowels of 
the three may be high ones. The combinations heard are as follows: 



70 Workpapers in Papua New Guinea Languages 



f 


e 


a 


G i 


i a 


a 


i 


o 





Q 


i 


i 


a 


1 


c i 


i u 


u 


e 


i 


3 


U 


i 


i 


a 


u 


G i 


1 o 


u 


e 


a 


3 


u 


a 


e 


a 


i 


a i 


i a 


u 


a 


i 


3 


u 





e 


a 


u 


a J 


t u 


u 


a 


u 









When four vowels come together they consist of arrangements of only 
three of the vowels (/i u a/). At least two of the vowels must be high 
vowels, but are never adjacent to each other. The combinations heard are 



as follows: 

a i a i 
a u Q u 
tola 

As discussed above in Section 3.2.4 the high vowels always are 
pronounced with the preceding phoneme, generally as the off-glide of the 
preceding vowel with total timing of the entire vowel sequence approaching 
the timing of two unambiguous vowels. 

7.4 Distribution of phonemes in words 

All the consonants occur in initial, medial and final syllables of 
words. All the vowels occur in initial, medial and final syllables of 
words In both V and CV configurations. There is no restriction on the 
location of any phoneme within a word. 

8. Morphophonemics 

8.1 Review of areas already discussed 

One area of morphophonemics has been discussed above in Section 6.2 
regarding phonetic consonant clusters across morpheme boundaries. This 
problem is the chief ongoing concern left in morphophonemic analysis. 

Another area already mentioned {Section 3.1.1) discussed word-final 
vowel loss when the following word began with a vowel which was exactly the 
same or very similar phonetically to the final vowel of the preceding word. 
This also occurred when the consonant beginning the next word was exactly 
the same or very similar phonetically to the last consonant of the 
preceding word. 

In Section 3.2.4 sequences of three or more vocolds were discussed. 
Some verb roots, however, end in the same vocoid as the attaching suffix 
and should create geminates or y-insertlon. It appears that geminates of 
the high vocolds collapse into one, but other geminates are separated with 
a semivowel. The collapsed geminates create two definitions for one 
phonetic utterance. 

/*■!/ 'go/recently went* [*■!] < {«i-i} 

go nr.past 

/'■opu/ 'stand/will stand' ['■opu] < {nopu -u ) 

stand fut 



Hewa 



71 



/■03o'Iaya/ 'is disposing of [Hopo'la^ya] < {«opo*la -a) 

dispose. of cont 

/mul u ' onoyo/ ' repeatedly asks' [bu lu ' anoyo] < (wulu ' ano-o) 

ask habitual 



8.2 Clitics 

There is one more category of phonological change which occurs. The 
pitch pattern changes when clitics are attached to either end of a word. 
The normal pitch pattern (Section 4.3.1) was described as having highest 
pitch on the first syllable and following syllables being at a lower pitch, 
the word terminating with a fading, falling pitch before silence. 

8.2.1 Contraction of the first person singular pronoun 

Often the first person singular pronoun /'ana/ 'I' is contracted to 
/a/, loses its stress, and becomes phonologically bound to the beginning of 
the next word. Though the combination of contraction and word sounds like 
one new word, the word to which the contraction attaches still retains its 
normal stress pattern. For example. 



/*ana 'mou/ ['ana 'aa"] 
I eat . future 



'I will eat' (Full form pattern) 



/a * mau/ 



[a'aau] 



(Frequent contraction pattern) 



It sounds as if the stress has shifted to the second syllable, but in 
reality, the word 'eat' has not changed. Representing the combined words 
phonemically as one word will allow the word 'eat' {or other verb) to 
retain its normal pronunciation, but it will still be distinguishable from 
/'aiiau/ ['ona"] 'food' by the stressed syllable. 

8.2.2 Vocative 



The same type of thing occurs in the pitch pattern when the vocative 
marker /o/ is added as a suffix to a word or message being spoken across 
the room or called across a greater distance, and is intended to draw the 
hearer's attention. Some examples follow. 

/'ycnc/ 'son' /'ycnco/ 'Son, . . . ' 

/ ' i na i / ' daughter ' / ' i na i o/ ' Daughter , . . . ' 

/ ' ap i g/ 'male name ' / ' api ao/ 'Oh, Apia, ... ' 

The pitch on the added final syllable does not fall and fade, but carries 
on at the unstressed syllable pitch level. 

In the following examples the contrast is the pitch level only. In the 
first sentence the /o/ is integral to the word meaning; in the second it is 
at the utterance (sentence) level of meaning. 



72 Workpapers in Papua New Guinea Languages 



/'ana 'm\o/ 'I often go' 

I go. habitual 

/'ana 'Bio/ 'I'm letting you know that I'm going now.' 
I go-voc .marker 

I propose using the space to indicate the levelling of the pitch by 
regarding the vocative marker as an unstressed word, the same as was 
proposed In Section 6.2. 

e.g. "Ana mio." vs. "Ana mi o." 

I go. habitual I go voc. marker 

8,2.3 Conjunctions 

When items are together in a series, a suffix is added to each item 
and affects the pitch pattern of the word to which it is attached, the same 
as mentioned In Section 8.2.2 immediately above. Nouns or clauses may be 
Joined. The space will again be used to indicate the phonological change Ln 
pitch pattern. An example follows. 

/'napa ki'awApA wcpc so'saiapa wcpc 'aliou wcpe 
we . pi KiawApA and Sosalapa and Al iau and 

*nopa 'ote ' uwe se *ycl«c/ 

we.pl river bank at go . upstream. direction .past 

•We (KiawApA, Sosalapa, Allau and I) went upstream along 
the river's bank.' 

8 . 3 Verbs 

All Hewa verb roots end in a vowel. When tense and aspect markers 
beginning with a high vowel (or semi-vowel) are added as suffixes to verb 
roots ending In /a/, they cause the /o/ to glide. The examples shown below 
are represented phonetically to Illustrate this point. 

Far Past Near Past Future Past 
Root Present Realis Realls Irrealis Completed 

/-0/ /-a-e/ /-I-c/ /-u-o/ /-po-a-c/ 



*'a" ' ao ' aoae 'sa^yc 'ma"w3 
eat 



aapoae 



-le'a- aele'a acle'aac ecle'a'yc acIe'auwD acle'cpoac 
return 

-*na- MA'na sa' naac aA'noiyc «a 'nauwo ba 'napoac 
hit, beat 

-•an I no- 'aonlna 'aaninoac 'aanlnaiyc 'aanlnauwo 'aaninapoac 



Hewa 73 



All other vowels at the end of a verb root do not change because the 
meaning is tied to the vowels. 

-'uwa^- 'nuwo* 'auffa'ac 'Muwafyc 'auiraiHo 'muwa'poac 

fill 

- ' aneo"- '■anea^' 'iBanea"»c *«anea"yc * aaneauirD '■anea"po«c 
hand over 

- ' 3- 'bo ' momc ' moyc ' ■owo ' bdpobc 

chop, carve 

- ' DU- • Bou 'aDuac ' Mouyc ' bouko ' aaupoac 
descend 

-tu'ono- 'aulu'ano aulu'cnoKe mulu'anoyc Bulu'cnowo mu I u ' anopoac 

Because no other vowels glide when morphemes are suffixed to the verb 
roots, but the roots retain their shape, these morphophonemic glides on /a/ 
will not be written, but the basic root form will be kept. This is subject 
to later testing when there are Hewa literates. (Cochran 1977: 91-92). 

Three verbs have been found to follow another pattern when only the 
future tense vowel suffix /u/ is added to the root. The last vowel is 
dropped and replaced by the /u/. These verbs return to the pattern when 
additional suffixes are added. The expected form is given last in the 
following examples . 

/' mcku/ 'will sleep' < (acki -u } ( */'mcki u/ ) 

sleep fut 

/'mckiwD/ 'intend to sleep' < {mcUi -u -a} 

sleep fut irrealis 

/■«ctu/ 'will build' < {mcti -u) (*/'«ctiu/; 

build fut 

/'mcliwD/ 'intend to build' < {acti -u -o ) 

build fut irrealis 

/ao'pu/ 'will put' < {ao'po-u) (*/ao'pou/) 

put fut 

/ao'powo/ 'intend to put' < {»o'po-u -3} 

put fut irrealis 

The phonemic shapes do not have the same form as any other known verb 
root, so there does not seem to be any confusion with other verbs. Also, 
later transfer of the phonemic shapes to an orthography wil' probably cause 
fewer errors for new readers than might spellings of the morphemic shapes 
on the right. Therefore, I prefer to use the phonemic shapes as the 
exceptions to the rule, rather than the morphemic shapes . This , too, is 
subject to later testing. 



Workpapers in Papua Kew Guinea Languages 
Volume 31 



Five E>l:^ono3. osr J- <=SLi S-t:\aca±GS3 



Summer Institute of Linguistics 
Ukarumpa, Papua New Guinea