Skip to main content

Full text of "Studies in Philippine Linguistics"

See other formats


THE PHONOLOGY OP SIBUTU SAMA: A LANGUAGE 
or THE SOUTHERN PHILIPPINES 

E* J. Allison 
Summer Institute of Linguistics 
Philippine Branch 



. Introducti on 

1. Presentation and Arrangement of the Data 

2. Suspect Data 

3. Phonemes 

1+. The Syllable 

5. The Phonological Word 

Footnotes 

Appendix: Tables 3-T and Text 



0, Introduction 

The speech of Sihutu Island in the Philippine Sulu Archipelago is 
one of several closely related languages and dialects comprising 
Southern Sama, a language family extending from Tanduhas Island (in the 
Tawi-Tavl group) south through the remaining islands of the archipelago 
and west as far as the northeast coast of Borneo. ^ Southern Sama 
itself is a member of the Sama-Bajaw subgroup of the Austronesian 
language family. 2 Speakers of Sibutu dialect nximber about 10,000, 
almost 13^ of the approximately 78,000 speakers of Southern Sama. ^ 

1, Presentation and Arrangement of the Data 

The sound system of Sibutu Sama is summarized in the phone chart of 
Table 1.^ A number of phones occxir there which are not ordinarily 
encountered in other Philippine languages, notably the voiced implosive 
stops and the lengthened contoids and vocoids. 

Circled segments show the allophones which are grouped within 
phonemes by the analysis. The suprasegmentals shown on the chart are 
only discussed incidentally in this paper, because they have not yet 
been fully investigated. All of the suprasegmentals shown are phonemic 
except stress, which is noncontrastive. 



6k The Phonology of Sibutu 



CONTOIDS 



STOPS 

vl 
vl unreleased 
vl lengthened 

vd 
vd implosive 
vd lengthened 
FLAP 

vd 
AFFRICATES 
vl grooved 
vd grooved 
FRICATIVE 

vl 

vd 
vl grooved 

vl grooved lengthened 
RESQNANTS 
vd lateral 
lengthened lateral 
nasal 
nasal lengthened 



¥ 





s 




s 




1 




1 


m 


n 


m: 


n 



dz^ 



V 

s 



BilaBial 


Alveolar 


Retro- 


Alveo 


Velar Glottal 






flexed Palatal 








Alveo- 










lar 






P 


t 






k q 


Pl 


t1 






kl 


p: 


t: 






k: 


h 


d 






g 


l' 


d^ 






< 
g 


-b: 


d: 

V 

r 






g: 



8 



n 



Table la. Phonetic Chart of Sibutu Sanaa 



MID OPEN 



LOW CLOSE 



The Phonology of Sihutu 65 



70C0IDS 



FRONT 



CENTRAL 



BACK 



HIGH CLOSE* 



HIGH OPEN 



I5I: 



u»u: 







MID CLOSE 



e,e: 



0,0: 



LOW OPEN 



a.a: 



SUPRASEOENTALS 



STRESS 



JUNCTUREl+J PAUSE OR SILEHCEC#] 



Primaiy ['] 
Secondary ["] 



Pitch levels [l],l2],l3] 
Terminal contours [+], C+l^Cl] 



Table lb. Phonetic Cb€urt of Sihutu Sama. 



* High close vocoids realize the high vowels in the syllable nucleus 
as well as the send vowels in non-nuclear syllable positions. 



66 The Phonology of Sibutu 

2. Suspect Data 

It is necessary to establish the statiis of foiir types of siispect 
segments and seqiiences: complex phones, high vocoids, lengthened seg- 
ments, and word-stem initial consonant clusters. 

2,1 Complex phones 

There are three ccarrplex phones contained in the phonetic data: 
[dz, s, n^]. The analysis has shown [s] to be an allophone of /s/ . 
(See Section 3 for a description of the conditioning environment.) The 
status of [dz] and [n^] requires further discussion. 

2.1.1 [dz] 

The voiced alveopalatal grooved affricate [dz] is interpreted as a 
xanit phoneme for the following reasons : 

(1) It occxirs word-stem initially as a syllable onset, and no consonant 
clusters function in this way in the nonsuspect data. Only single seg- 
ments may fill onset and coda positions. Examples of [dz] word-stem 
initially are as followsi 

['dza.a] 'a k.o. confection' 
[dz.u. '^a.lan] 'fried bananas' 
['dzan.tan] 'ugly' 
[ ' dza. lair] • seldom' 

(2) It occurs with other consonants in clusters, and the nonsuspect data 
do not permit more than two consonants in a single cluster. Por example: 

['lon.dzok] 'raise the hand' 
[bal.'dzan. dziq] 'Muslim prayer book* 

(3) It occurs as a geminate cluster, in which each member must function 
as a single segment. Examples are: 

['had.dz'q] /'haj.J'q/ 'title of Mecca pilgrim' 
['bad.dz'uq] /'baj.juq/ 'clothes' 

The phonetic realization of /jj/ as [dd^] is discussed in Section 2.3.1.1)* 

2.1.2 n^ 

The voiced alveopalatal nasal In^] is also interpreted as a unit 
phoneme, becaxise it freely occurs in the syllable onset position. The 
interpretation of the feature of palatalization as a vowel is to be re- 
jected, since this featvure never occurs with syllabicity. 



The Phonology of Sibutu 6? 



2.2 High Vocoids 



The high vocoids ^ij ^J Q-^e interpreted as vowels /i,u/ when they 
occur as syllable pesiks and as consonants /yjw/ when they occur else- 
where. A description of the environments in which they occur follows: 



2.2.1 Environment of the High Vocoids 

(1) Between consonants 
['si.p^tl] 'clip' ['bu.lsuti] 
['sig.pi^tl] 'tight' ['bun.kuki] 'humpbacked' 

(2) Between a consonant and silence (i.e., jimcture or pa\ise)< 



'back J dorsal' 



['ka*li] 'dig' ['ha.lu] 

['i.li] 'there' ['u.lan] 

(3) Clustered with a low vocoid. 

a) adjacent to a consonant. 

['si.ag] 'tube-like skirt' ['pu.as ] 

[ si . ' a. ka] ' older sibling ' [ 'pu. ' a. sa] 
['ke.itl ] 'safety pin' 

b) adjacent to silence. 
' orphan ' 
'why?' 
'past particle' 



['SA.Ukl] 



[' a.t^m] 
['qa^ ] 



.u 



[be^ ] 



['"akl.tu] 
['b^^.lu] 



['ta^.ta^] 'to cross abridge' fta.lA^] 
['pa.te^ ] 'kill' ['sa.g^^ ] 

{k) Clxistered with each other 

a) adjacent to a consonant 

['ti.upT] 'to blow' ['hu.is] 

b) adjacent to word-final silence 

[ba. 'li.u] 'wind' ['tu.ij 

c) adjacent to word-initial silence 



^incite' 



'rain' 



['^ukl] 



'say' 



.u 



[' i.li] 



'after' 
'fasting' 

'dipper' 

' time ' 

'odor' 

'k.o. rice cake' 

'to fear' 

'busy' 



'Judge' 
'directly' 
'personal name' 



68 The Phonology of Sihutu 



(5) Between vocoids 



a) Between nonsuspect vocoids 

['da. an] 'princess' ['la. ari] 'door' 

[ ' lo . okl ] ' rattle ' [ ' ta. al] ' incantation ' 

b) between other vocoids 

['gu. ud] 'drag' [''ba, i. 'ba. i] 'epilepsy' 
['ka. u ] 'tree' ['tA .ua ] 'correct' 
['ku. a ] 'monkey' ['da. un] 'oar' 

2.2.2 Interpretation of the High Vocoids 

In environments (l) and (2) above, the high vocoids are sLLways 
syllabic, f;mctioning as syllable peaks, and so are interpreted as 
vowels. This is widely supported in nonsuspect data. 

['da.patl] 'opportunity' ['be. lad ] 'dirty-eyed' 
['lan.katl] 'dismantle' pben.ten] 'carry by hand' 

['da. las] 'extravagant' ['b^n.to! ] 'straight' 

['ba.ha ] 'shoulder' ['a.bas ] 'skin rash' 

In environment C3a) the high vocoids are interpreted as vowels also, 
since (l) they are syllabic, (2) they may occur with primary stress, and 
(3) nonsuspect data do not admit a CC cluster in the same syllable. 
Vowel clusters of the type in CSa) are also well supported l^y non- 
suspect data. 

['de.oq] 'under' ['ko.aki] 'ground depression' 

[ 'be.aq] 'accompany' [he. 'a.na] 'its greatness' 
['bo.'aq.na] 'his mounth' 

In environment (3b) the high vocoids are never syllabic. When any 
of the forms ending in a high vocoid are suffixed (whether the stem is 
one or more syllables) the primary stress is not permuted to the high 
vocoid, as it would be if the vocoid were syllabic. For example 
['hiw ] 'c?dor' becomes ['bA .han] 'bad odor', not *[bA 'u.han]; 
['ta . ta^] 'to cross a bridge' becomes ["pa.ta . !ta .an] 'place of 
crossing'. If the high vocoid in a word like ['ta .ta ] were syllabic, 
the rioles of stress would require the stem to be stressed as 
*["ta.i. 'ta.i] . Therefore, the high vocoids in'this enrironment are 
interpreted as consonants. Other examples are: 

[' a.t^m] 'orphan' [' akl.tu] 'time' 



The Phonology of Sibutu 69 

[^qa^] 'why?» [»bA^] 'odor' 

['ta .ta ] *to cross a [*ta.lA ] *feax' 

bridge* 

In (Ua) and (Ub) the high vocoids are interpreted as vowels, since 
they always function as syllable peaks • Vowel cliisters in this 
environment are supported by nonsxaspect data. 

[ 'me . on] • cat ' [ »ke . atl] 'red' 

[»bo.a] 'carry' ["pa. ke* 'a, tun] 'make it red' 

In (Uc) the high vocoids are nonsyllabic and are interpreted as 
consonants: 

[' uk] 'say' [ ili] 'personal name' 

This is analogous to the interpretation of (3b) where nonsyllabic high 
vocoids cl\ister with low vocoids word-initially. 

In environment (5) the high vocoids are interpreted as consonants, 
inasmuch as (l) they do not function syllabically, (2) never carry 
stress, and (3) there is no support in nonsxispect data for a three-vowel 
cluster. The nonsuspect data, of co\irse, widely support the occurrence 
of intervocalic consonants. 

2.3 Lengthened Segments 

All vowels and all consonants except /n /, /q/and /h/ occur with 
phonemic length. Such lengthened segments are interpreted as geminate 
clusters. The reasons for this interpretation are given in the follow- 
ing discussion. 

2.3.1 Lengthened Consonants 

Nons\ispect data do not permit the interpretation of any consonant 
cl\ister as occurring within the same syllable. The distribution of con- 
sonant clusters is such that they always occur across a syllable^ bo\indary , 
the first member as a syllable coda, and the last member as a syllable 
onset. Such clusters do not occur word finally, but they do occiir word 
initially. (See section 2.U.) Inasmuch as lengthened consonants follow 
the same rules as nonsuspect consonant clusters, they are interpreted as 
two identical segments, i.e., geminate clusters. The distribution of 
consonant clusters in nonsuspect data is shown below, along with some 
examples of geminate clxisters. 

['han.taklj 'fall hard' J'brd.dakl] 'face powder' 

['dag. tuq] 'to yank' ['del.laq] 'tongue' 

['sig.p^^tlj 'tight' [*l^l,P*Pi.tl] 'carry under arm' 



70 The Phonology of Sibutu 



[^srua.dai] * enduring' ['hhn.nal] 'true' 

There are many exanrples of mnimal contrast between geminate 

clusters and single segments of the same type as that making up the 
clxister. A few are shown here. 

['ka.pal] 'thick' [*k^p.pal] 'ship' 

['be.teq] 'similar' ['bet.teq] 'to trip' 

['ba.sa] 'because' ['bNS.sa] 'to read' 

['ha. la] 'defeated' ['hid. la] 'hiisband' 

['ta.naq] 'ground' ['tAn. naq.] 'put' 



2.3.1.1 The Lengthened Affricate 

The phonetic sequence [ddz] is interpreted as /jj/. This follows 
Pallesen's^ interpretation of the same phenomenon in the Siasi language, 
and for the same reasons: (l) [dz] is a confilex phone, an affricate 
with [d] as the first member, and phonetic realization of /Jj/ is highly 
probable as [ddz]; and (2) [d] does not occur elsewhere internal to a 
word stem as the first member of a consonant cluster, except when it 
occurs as a geminate cluster. In addition, therg is one example in^the 
Sibutu Sama data of minimal ^contrast between [ddz] and [dz]: ['ad.dzal] 
'to prepare food' and ['a.dzal] 'to preach a sermon'. Other exanples 
of [ddz] and [dz] are: 

['bad.dzuq] 'baj. juq/ 'clothes' 

['had.dziq]/'haj.jiq/ 'title of Mecca pilgrim' 

["ka.dza.'lang.ki^n] /"ka. ja. 'lang.'kiri/ 'scorpion' 

[lad. 'dzi-kiq] /laj . ' ji.kiq/ 'material blessing' 
["ka.la. 'dza.qan]/"ka.la. ' ja.qan/ 'behaviour' 

2.3.2 Lengthened Vowels 

Vowels occurring with phonemic length are also interpreted as 
geminate clusters. This interpretation is based on the following con- 
siderations. 

(1) The nonsuspect data do not give evidence for any type of complex 
vocoid as syllable peak. 

(2) All nonsiispect vocoid clusters are distributed across syllable 
boundaries. 

(3) The interpretation leads to a symmetrical phonemic system with both 
vowels and consonants occurring in geminate clusters. 



The Phonology of Sihutu 71 

Some exanqples of minimal contrast follow. 

['boq] 'jxist now' ['bo-oq] 'bamboo' 

['ba] 'exclamatoiy particle' ['ba. a] 'embers' 

f'bu.ku] 'knot' [bu. 'u.ku] ^wy hair' 

['ka.mi] 'we (neutral focus) [ka. 'mi.i] 'we (topic focus) 

['e^.le] 'that, far' [e.'e^.le] 'that, very 

remote ' 

2.i* Word-Initial Consonant Clusters 

Homorganic and geminate consonant clxisters occur word-stem 
initially in the phonetic data. Exan4)les are: 



['p.paq] 


•address to 


father' 


[•t.tuci] 


'address to 


older brother' 


I'k.kaq] 


•address to 


older sibling or cousin' 


[•b^.byn] 


•porpoise' 




['d^.d;»pl] 


•nightmare' 




['g\gai:l] 


' belch • 




['l.lKw] 


'sun, day' 




E'm.lDai] 


•no' 




[•n.da] 


•wife^ 




I'n.gacJ 


'mother' 





In phonetic sequences of this type, the first member of the cluster 
is interpreted as a syllable peak, and the second member as the onset of 
the following syllable. The reasons for this interpretation are as 



follows : 



r 



(1) There are no nons\ispect examples of a consonant cluster filling an 
onset or coda position. Therefore, when a consonant cluster occurs 
between vowels word-medially, it must be analyzed as falling across 
syllable boundaries. It follows that this same interpretation ought 
to be made when a consonant cluster occurs word-stem initially. 

(2) When a cliister of this type occurs word-stem initially, the first 
member of the cluster takes primary word stress if it is in penul- 
timate position, jtist as does any vowel-peaked syllable in the same 
position. The stress then permutes to the penultimate syllable 
when a suffix is added, precisely the same as in vowel-peaked 
syllables. 



72 The Phonology of Sibutu 

['m.boq] 'grandmother' [m. *boq.ku] 'my grandmother' 

I'n.daJ 'wife* [n, 'da.ku] 'my wife' 

For a similar phenomenon in the Siganggang dialect of Siasi Sama, 
Pallesen has postulated the presence of a phonemic schwa before the word- 
stem initial consonant cl\ister. However, for Sibutu Sama this inter- 
pretation must be rejected for the reasons given in points 3 through 5, 
following. 

(3) There are no phonetic data in Sibutu Sama indicating the pi*esence of 
a phonemic vowel. The first member of a consonant cluster in word- 
stem initial position is uttered by a native speaJier without a pre- 
ceding vowel sound. For example, [m] and the implosive [b<] are 
sounded with the lips closed. Initial [p] is formed silently with 
the lips closed, the second [p] being released as the onset of the 
next syllable. 

(U) When a consonant-final prefix occurs before these clusters, only a 
very minimal transition sound, and frequently no sound at all, 
occurs between the affix and the cluster. Native speakers will not 
accept a perceivable vowel sound in this position. "For example, 
if the prefix [mag-J ('distributive action with fociis on actors') 
is added to ['g<.gskl3 ('belch') the resulting (full) form is 
[mAg. 'ga*itan] 'they are belching'. The implosive allophone is lost 
leaving only the geminate cluster [gg] without any intervening 
vowel soimd." 

(5) When a stem-initial consonant cluster is preceded by a consonant 
which has an intervocalic allophone, the allophone does not occ\ir, 
as would be expected if a phonemic vowel.were present in initial 
position on the stem. For example ['ma. ad m. 'boq.ku] 'my grand- 
mother, will pay' does not have the [r] allophone of [d] as in 
*['ma."^ar ^m. 'boq.ku] . This latter utterance is rejected by a 
Sibutu speaker. (in the language investigated by Pallesen, the 
allophone does occur. ) 



3» Phonemes 

The phonemic inventory of Sibutu Sama consists of twenty-two seg- 
mentsil phonemes, seventeen consonants and five vowels. They are 
identified in the phoneme chart of Table 2. 



The Phonology of Sibutu 73 



Table 2. Phonemes of Sibutu Sama 

CONSONANTS 

ALVEO 
BILABIAL ALVEOLAR PALATAL VELAR 



GLOTTAL 



STOPS 






Simple: 






vl 


P 


t 


vd 


b 


d 


Affricated: 






FRICATIVES 






vl 






vl grooved 




s 


RESONANTS 






Lateral 




1 


Nasal 


m 


n 


SEMIVOWEL 


w 





k 
g 



ng 



TOWELS 



PRONT 



CENTRAL 



BACK 



HIGH 

MID 
LOW 



u 



3«1« Consonants 

Tables 3 throvigh 5 deinonstrate the phonemic status of the consonants. 
(Table T is an English index to the vords used in these tables.) The 
phonemes /h/ and /rr / do not occixr in word- final position. Neither does 
the phoneme /J/, except in borrowed words (e.g. /haj/ 'pilgrimage to 
Mecca'). The glottal stop /q/ occurs in all positions, but it is 
Gontrastive only word medi€J.ly and word-finally, having lost its phonemic 
status word initially. The remaining consonants contrast in all 
positions. 

3.1.1 /p,t,k/ 

The voiceless stops /p,t,k/ myay be considered together, inasmuch as 
they have common distinctive properties, and the conditioning environment 
for their allophones is identical. 



7^ The Phonology of Sibutu 

The phoneme /p/ is a bilabial with an unreleased allophone [pi] 
and a slight-to-zero aspirated allophone [p] ; phoneme /t/ is an 
alveolar with an unreleased allophone [tl] and a slight-to-zero 
aspirated allphone [t] ; and the phoneme /k/ is a velar with an unreleased 
allophone [kl] and a slight-to-zero aspirated allophone [k] . 

The unreleased allophones [pl,tl,kl] occur only in the syllable 
coda position, either (l) preceding pause /#/, (2) before a consonant 
in the same word, or (3) preceding word juncture /+/ in the same pause 
group. In the latter case, when the following word begins with a 
consonant, [pl^tljkl] invariably occxir; however, when the following 
word begins with a vocoid [p1,t1,kll vary freely with [p,t,k] repectively. 

The allophones [p,t,k] occur as syllable onset in all environments. 
As Just noted, they also occur as syllable coda before a vocoid in a 
following word, varying freely with unreleased [ pl,t1,kl]. The rule may 
be summarized as follows: 

r (c) V _/#/ 

{ p1,tl,kl] / J (C) v_cv(c) 

L (c) v_/+/c 

/p,t,k/-* \ [pT,tl,k1]'v-[p,t,k] / (C) V_/+/V 

[p , t ,k ] /elsewhere 

Following are examgples illustrating the conditioning environments of the 
voiceless stops. 

['ta.pa] ^broil' 

["ta.ka. 'ta.kal 'continually' 

[ * tapi. tapl] * steadfast ' 

[' sati . satT ] ' distraction ' 

['dakl.dakll 'vash clothes' 

[be""" 'la.tapl ma "sam.bu.an .an]'there wa^ a flood in Zamboanga. ' 

['la.qipl'v'la.qip 'a.nu 'na.anl'that one is stupid' 

[♦ke.atlna "bu.aq'bu.aq 'e.lej'that frxiit is already red' 

[^ke.atl'x^'ke.at 'a.nuna.an] 'that one is red.' 

['lN.ukl,ma.ka 'bu.as ] 'viand and rice' 

['IN .ukT^^'lA.iik a. 'nu.na] 'his viand' 



The Phonology Of Sihutu 75 



3.1.2 /b.d,g/ 

The only voicing contrast in the phonological system occtjts between 
the voiced and voiceless stops. Like their voiceless counterparts , the 
voiced stops /b^djg/ are most economically described together. 

The phoneme /b/ is a bilabial with voiced fricative [l>] voiced 
inrplosive [b ], and voiced stop [b] allophones. 

The phoneme /d/ is an alveolar with voiced flap [r] , voiced 
iirrplosive [d ], and voiced stop [d] allophones. 

The phoneme /g/ is a velar with voiced fricative [-c] i voiced 
implosive [g ], and voiced stop [g] allophones. 

The voiced fricatives [te,g] and the voiced flap [r] occur inter- 
vocalically, either within a word or at word boundaries. In these 
positions [te,g] alternate freely with [b,g]; [r] however is not in free 
alternation with [d] . 

The voiced implosives [b ,d ,g ] occ^ only as the first memTjer of 
a word-initial geminate cluster, as in ^b .bong] 'poipoise', ["d -d^p] 
'nightmare' and [g .gak] 'belch'. 

The voiced stop allophones [b,d,g] occur elsewhere. The rule is 
summarized a^: 



/■b.g/ 



[te,g]'\'lb,g]/V_V 
< Il)^,g'']/+.#/_yT3»g/ respectively 
[ ["b.d.g ] /elsewhere 
" [r] / V_V 



I 



/d/ i [d^]/+/_/d/ 

[d /elsewhere ^ 

Exaii5)les follow for the voiced stops 



I'a.bu]'V'['a.bu] 'ashes' 
['si.«a]'v/['si.6aj 'plural' 
['ha. ran] 'hody' 

I'b*^.bun] 'porpoise' 

£• gad. dun] 'green' 



['g .gakl] 'belch' 

[ ' d^ . d>p1 ] ' nightmare ' 

j;'ha.g^^d]'\'['ha.g(^d] 'match' 
I'dag.tuq] 'yank' 

'I'geh.hel] 



fat waist' 



T6 The Phonology of Sibutu 



3.1.3 /ci/ 

The phoneme /q/ i 
in both syllable onset 
limitation being that 
initial position, [q] 
any other phoneme, but 
q varies freely with 
another word that ends 
ending in a consonant. 



s realized as the glottal stop [q] . It occiirs 

and coda positions, its only distributional 
it does not form geminate clusters. In the word- 
does not enter into a distinctive opposition with 
in order positions it functions contrast ively. 
its absence when it occurs word-initially following 
in a vowel. Following pause, or following a word 
it occurs invariably. 



The rule is as follows: 



/q/ 



Examples are: 






[q.M^]/ V/+/ 

ic/+/_v 



V 



J 



[*min.ga 'qa.nu ma *qa.ku?]'v 
i'min.ga 'a.nu ma *a,ku?] 
['s^l.li qa. 'ku.u 'no. bet 'qi.yaj'v^ 
['s^l.li a. 'ku.u'no.bet 'qi.ya] 
['qa.ku ni dJfam. 'ba.tan] 



'where is mine?' 

'I love to pinch her' 
'I'm going to the wharf 



3.1.H /J/ 

The phoneme /j/ is realized as a voiced alveopalatal grooved 
affricate, [d5f] , which occurs in both onset and coda positions. As 
coda, [dz] does not cluster with any other consonant within a word 
stem except a geminate in the following syllable. [dz] does not occur 
in word- final position except in borrowed words (e.g. [hadif] 'pilgrimage 
to Mecca' ). 

The following examples illustrate the distribution of [dz] . 



['dzaji.dXiq] 
[baJL. 'dzan.dziq] 
['sid.dza]/rsij.ja/ 
[•mas.dz^^d] 



'promise' 



'Muslin prayer book' 

' only ' 

'mosque' 



The Phonology of Sibutu 77 



3.1.5 /h/ 

The phoneme /h/ is realized as a voiceless pharyngeal spirant, ([ hj, 
which occiirs freely in the syllable onset position. It does not occur 
in a consonant cl\ister within a word stem, nor does it occur word finally 
except in borrowed terms (as in L'al.lahj *godM» 



Examples are: 



['ha. la] 

[•hal.la] 

['hu.^^s] 

['ba.hanj 

I'ko.hal] 



'defeat' 
'husband' 
'judge a case' 
'vine' 



'cough' 



3.1.6 /s/ 

V 

The phoneme- /s/ has a voiceless grooved alveopalatal allophone [s] 
and a voiceless grooved alveolar allophone [s] . The [s] allophone varies 
freely with [s] in unstressed syllables preceding /ia/. The [s] allo- 
phone occurs elsewhere. Thus: 



/s/ 



\sJa.[sJ / _/i.'aC/ 



> 



[s*] / elsewhere 



Examples are as follows: 

[si. 'a.ka]'^[^i. 'a.ka] 
Isi. 'a.lij'^lsi. 'a.lij 
Isi. 'a.pu]'^[si. 'a.pu] 
[pa. 'si.al] 
I 'si. am] 

I'sa^liqJ 
I'ba. sa] 
I'ka.tas] 



'older sibling' 
■younger sibling' 
'waterspout' 
'stroll' 



nine' 



'same ' 

'becaiise' 
'paper' 



78 The Phonology of Sibutu 



3.1.7 /I/ 

The phoneme /l/ is a resonant with a voiced retroflexed alveolar 
lateral allophone [1] and a voiced alveolar lateral allophone [l] . The 
retroflexed lateral [1] occurs only as a syllable coda (l) before paiise, 
or (2) before a consonant other than /l/ in the same vord stem or 
(3) before a consonant in the folloving vord. It never occurs as a 
syllable onset. The non-retroflexed alveolar lateral [l] occurs freely 
as syllable onset. As syllable coda it occurs only as the first member 
of a geminate cluster, or in word- final position before an initieil 
vocoid in the following word. The following rule applies: 



/I/ 



(C)Y_^#/ 

Cc)v_c,c?^ /I/ L y 
(c)v_/+/c 



/ elsewhere 



Examples : 

[^la.latl] 'lightning' 

[ ' la. pal] ' utterance ' 

[ ' bal - 1 ^^tl ] ' reversed' 

['a.ne^ 'pal. pal a. 'ku.u] 'why, am I deaf? 

I'm. bal du sab] 'certainly not. 

3.1.8 /m,n,ng,n^/ 

The nasal resonant phonemes /m, n, ng, n^/ are realized as [m, n, 
n, n^], respectively. [m] is a voiced bilabial nasal; [n] a voiced 
alveolar nasal; [n] a voiced velar nasal; and [n^] a voiced alveopalatal 
nasal, [m, n, n] occur freely in the syllable onset and coda positions. 
[n^J occurs only in the syllable onset position and does not form con- 
sonant clusters. 



Examples : 



[ma] 

['lu.mu] 

['ll^m.miq.] 



'at' 

'tired' 

'dirty' 



[ 'ka. am] 
I'lam.batl] 
Ina ] 



'you (pi. ) ' 

'slow' 
'now' 



The Phonology of Sibutu 79 



[ ^ha.qan] 

I 'lan.duqj 
I 'no.betl] 
I^ka.nag] 

[pa, *n^a.am] 



'sneeze' { »ka.nan] 

'very' I'b^n.nalJ 

'is pinching' ['ba.an] 

'parrot' I'lan.katT] 

'irritable' Ibu. 'ha.n^ag] 'to spread' 

'pancake' I'n^a.^] 'soul, spirit' 



'left' 

'true' 

'if 

'to dismantle' 



3.1.9 /v,y/ 



The phonemes /v,y/ are realized as the vocoi 
I, [ ' J occur freely, but in the syllable ons 



ds [ ' ] • As syllable 
coda, L"'" J occur freely, but in the syllable onset position, they are 
limited in regard to the formation of consonant cloisters, occurring in 
geminate cloisters only. Examples include the following: 



[^ a-k^] 

[•kA^l 

L'bA^.kal] 

[' sA .mul] 
[' a.t^m] 

['se .tan] 



' agent ' 

'you' 

'bushes' 

' alvays ' 
' orphan ' 
'past particle' 
' demon ' 
'vocative' 



['ka. akl] 'groxmd depression' 



u 



['bA^.lu] 



u 



I'kA .sun] 
['tA . aqj 
['ko/^k7] 
['be^.la] 
['pe^.gaq] 



'k.o. rice cake' 
'brown' 
'correct ' 
'to rattle' 
'dance' 
'search for' 



3.2 Vowels 

The five vowel phonemes are /i, u, e, o, a/. Unlike many other 
Sama dialects, Sibutu Sama does not have the schwa phoneme. There are 
restrictions on the combinations of vowels that will form clusters, the 
permissible clusters being /iu, ia, ei , eo, ea, ui, ua, oa, au/. In 
addition, each vowel also enters into geminate clxister combinations 
(this being the interpretation given to phonemic length). The phonemic 
status of the vowels is demonstrated in Table 6 (see Appendix). 



3.2.1 /i/ 

The phoneme /i/ has two SLllophones: [J , a high open front 
unrcu .ded vocoid; and lij , a high close front imrounded vocoid. [J 
occurs in unstressed closed syllables (exgept where the onset or coda is 
filled by a glottal stop), and preceding homorganic and geminate con- 
sonant clusters followed by /i/;^ [i] occurs in all other environments. 
The rule is: 



80 The Phonology of sibutu 



/i/ 



Examples are as follows: 

[a. 'lis.tu] 
['bi.li^kl] 
['di.ki] 
[•di.ri] 
[*h|^l.l|^n] 
[•in. dam] 
{b(^s. 'sa.la] 
I'b^^s.si] 



10 I 

[i] / 



unstressed C ^C, where C ^ /q,/ 

/CCi, where CC = a homorganic or 

geminate cluster 

elsewhere 



' clever • 

'room* 

'little' 

'ugly' 

'sound' 

'borrow' 

'speak' 

'iron (metal)' 



3.2.2 /u/ 

The phoneme /u/ has two allophones: [u] , a high half-close back 
rounded vocoid; and [u] , a high close back rounded vocoid. £o] occurs 
in unstressed closed syllables (except where the onset or coda is filled 
by a glottal stop); and preceding homorganic and geminate consonant 
clusters followed by /u/; [u] occurs elsewhere. Thus: 

unstressed C C, where C ^ /q/ 



Jul 



Exainples are: 



['bug.golj 

I'bul.lod] 

J'lu.qun] 

['kud.datl] 

['ku.lanj 

[•bu.bus] 



Ml 

[u] / elsewhere 



/CCu/, where CC = a homorganic or 

geminate consonant cluster 



'stooped' I 'bud. Jan] 'maiden' 

'mountain ' J ' lun . su}] ' stroll ' 

'present at' ['bu.kotlj 'back' 

' surprised' [ 'ba. lu] 'widow' 



'lacking' 

'fall c3ff, as 
leaves' 



[* turn. pun J 'money collection 
box' 



The Phonologsr O^ Sibutu 8l 



3.2.3 /e/ 

The phoaaeiae /e/ has allophones Je] and £ej . [e] is amid half- 
close front xmroxoaded vocoid; and Je] is a mid close front unroomded 
vocoid. [e] occxirs in unstressed closed syllables (except vhere the 
onset or coda is filled hy a glottal stop); and preceding homorganic 
and geminant consonant clxisters followed Ify /e/; [e] occiirs elsevhere. 
Thus: 



/e/ 



U]/ 



unstressed C C, where C^ /q/ 

/CCe/, where CC = a homorganic or 

geminate consonant cluster 

le]/ elsewhere 



Examples are: 



[»be.teq] 
[^hel.leqj 
[•be. lad] 
[»bel»l3d] 



'similar' 
'sea eagle' 
'dirty-eyed' 
'slow-learner' 



['bet.teq] 'trip one's feet' 
['ben. ten] 'cany by hand' 
I ben. 'te.ni^n] 'cany it' 
['be.len] 'turn ajround' 

Ibe.'leT^-ku] 'I'll turn it' 



3.2. U /o/ 

The phoneme /o/ has allophones [o] and [o] . [o] is a mid half- 
close back rounded vocoid; and JoJ is a mid close back rounded vocoid. 
T ;>] occtirs in unstressed closed syllables (except where the onset or coda 
is filled by a glottal stop), and preceding homorganic and geminate 
consonant clusters followed by /o/; and (;o ] occurs elsewhere. Thus: 

unstressed C C, where C ^ /q./ 

[O] / ^^CCo/ where CC = a honorganic or 

/q/ geminate consonant cluster 

[o] / elsevhere 



Exajcples are: 



I'tok.k^d] 'guess' [»bo.b>k^] 

Xt;>k. 'ko.ran] 'guessing something' ['ha.lo] 

I"bDl.loq. 'bsLloqi] 'pretending' ['bon.n^d] 

I'd>n.d?n] 'stare' [HDm.pDl] 



•trash' 
•lagcKxi' 
'numb' 
'dull' 



82 The Phonology of Sibutu 



[*kom.pan] 'vaist' 



3.2,5 /a/ 

The phoneme /a/ has allophones [a] and [a]. [a] is a mid open 
central \inrounded vocoid; and [a] is a lov open central imrounded vocoid. 
[aJ occurs before /u/; and before /v/ followed by a consonant or silence. 
[a] etlso varies freely with [a] before homorganic and gejoodnate con- 
sonant cl\isters followed by /a/, [a] occxirs elsewhere. Thus: 



/a/ 



[A] / 



_/vC/ ,/^/ 



[A]*^[a] / /CCa/, where CC = a homorganic or 

geminate consonant cliister 

[a] / elsevhere 



Examples are: 



^kAs.saq.l'^t'kas.saq] *bottle* 

»bAd.daklJ'^'[*bad.daK] »faoe powder* 

' lAn . dan]*^ [ * Ian . dan] 

•b\n.ta]'^[*ban.ta] 

»kan.t^l] 

» tAn. daoJ'^r'tan. daqj 

* ban. tug] 

»dAn.dajM'danda3 

♦bN^.lu] 

'sN.uk ] 



'fry fish' 

'enemy' 

'bed' 

'sign' 

'well-known' 

'female' 



'k.o. rice cake' I'sa.pA^] 
'dipper' I'bA^.kal] 



['b^n.nalj'^t'han.nal] 'true' 
I'bAl.la(i]'^['bal.l8qJ 'cook' 
['gad. dun] 'green' 

C'tam^pDl] close x?)' 

I'lAl.la]'>'['lal»la] 'male' 
I'kAn,8an]'^['kan.6an] 'always' 
I'qan.damJ 'cloudy' 

[.Vg/»ga.tal]'^[gan#tal] 'rainbow' 

'roof 
'shrub' 



U. The Syllable 

Both vowels and consonants have been fotmd to function as syllabic 
peaks of resonance within the syllable in Sibutu Sama. Vowel-peaked 
syllables have unlimited distribution in the phonological word, but 
consonant-peaked syllables are limited to occurrence in stem-initxal 
word position. Certain vowel-peaked syllables may occur in isolation 
(e.g., single-syllable word forms), but a consoi^ant-peaked syllable 
never does. 



The Phonology of Sihutu 83 



U.l Vowel-Peaked Syllable 



There are four non-suspect syllable patterns: V, VC, CV and CVC. 
Exaaqples illustrating these in comhination are as follows: 



VC 


[ea] 


'sigentive particle' 


v.vc 


[•o.ekl] 


'yell' 


7.CV 


[•a.sa] 


'depend on' 


CV 


[ba] 


'certainly' 


cv.v 


[•bo. a] 


'carry' 


cv.vc 


[•bo.aqj 


'mouth' 


cv.cvc 


I'bo.baklJ 


'trash' 


cvc.cvc 


['la«r^.kat1] 


'dismantle' 


cv.cv.cv 


Ipa. 'ka.lel 


'listen' 



From these and similar eacanrples it can readily be seen that the 
vowel-peaked syllable consists of a vowel as syllable peak with optional 
onset and coda positions filled by consonants, i.e., (C)v(C). Syllables 
of this type may occur freely in any combination. A consonant that 
occurs intervocalically between two syllable peaiks is always the onset 
of the following syllable. When a consonant cluster occurs intervocal- 
lically, the first member of the cluster functions as a syllable coda 
and the second member as a syllable onset. For example: 

I'ka.palJ ^ thick' ['ke.sPg] 'strong' 

l%p.pal] 'ship' ["ke. 'so.g^^n] 'use strength' 

Any vowel may occur as a syllable peak and any consonant as a 
syllable onset. All the consonants except /r? / and /dz/ may also freely 
occTir as a syllable coda. The nasal alveopalatal /n^/ never occurs in 
coda position; and as observed earlier the voiced grooved affricate 
/dz/ occurs as coda only in borrowed words. 

In the non-siispect data, consonant clusters never function as 
unitary fillers of a single onset or coda position. 

U.2. Consonant-Peaked Syllable 

A syllable with consonant peak is posited for this language. Evi- 
dence supporting this interpretation is presented in section 2.k. Con- 
sonants that may occur as syllable peaks include the sinqple stops 
(except the glottal stop), the grooved fricative /s/, and the resonanta, 
except [n^] . Examples of words with syllables of this type are given 



&k The Phonology of Sihutu 



in section 2.3. Some additional exanrples follow: 

['m.maq] 'father' ['m.boqt] * mother' 
['n.doq] 'to see' ['ng.gey] 'don't' 
['s.sa ] 'taste' ['d.dzu] 'criticize' 

5. The Phonological Word 

The phonological word consists of one or more syllables bounded hy 
Jioncture, plus the occurrence of stress. Observe the following examples: 

(la) ['hu.nun + "kA .'ka.nat + '.anu + 'tu.tu] 'are these willing to 

be s cattered (i.e., 
separated)? ' 

Clb) I'hu.nun + 'kA + 'ka.nat + 'a.nu + 'tu.tu] 'are you willing (that) 

these scatter?' 

(2a) I'a.ne ^ + 'kA^-sun + 'tu.tu] 'why is this brown (and not some 

other color)?* 

C2b) I'a.ne^ + 'k\^ + sun. 't\ak.nu] 'why are you hitting yourself? 

Utterances (la) and (lb) contain identical syllables and near- 
identical stress patterns. However [kA ] 'you' is a separate morpheme 
in (lb), while in (la) it is the integral first syllable of [kA .'ka.nat] 
'scattered' (which cannot be divided further into morphemes). The 
difference, clearly discemable to a native speaker, appears to be a 
f\anction of both stress and the timing between the phonetic seqtiences. 



In (2a) and (2b) the same type of phenomenon occurs. Here the 
stress pattern and the number of syllables are identical. Yet again, 
I'kA^J 'you' is a separate morpheme in (2b), but cannot be separated 
from I'kAT.sunJ 'brown' in (2a). In these utterances both stress and 
the timing between words functions to keep words apart. The feature of 
timing is accounted for by positing the Juncture phoneme /+/. 

5.1 Jvmcture /+/ 

Jmicture in this language, unlike juncture in English, appears to 
be characterized primarily by time-lapse transition between words 
(rather than the complex of articulatory features plus timing as "crisp" 
versus "muddy" transitions and the like). The length of the time-lapse 
is probably conditioned by word stress, which has an effect on the 
length of the word. However, the exact nature of the relationship bet- 
ween Juncture and stress still requires investigation. 

It should be pointed out that the phonological word does not always 
coincide with the semantic fonn. Note the following: 



The Phonology of Sibutu 85 

(3a) [*i. a + pa. 'ra.gan + "ma.ka.lu, 'ma^anj *he runs and walks* 

(3b) I*i. a + "ma. ka. 'ra.gan +"ma.ka.lu. 'ma.anj *he can run and can 

valk' 

(3c) pi. a + pa. 'ra.gan + "ma.ka.lu. ' a.qan] 'he is running outside' 

In utterance (3a) the word {"ma.ka.lu. 'ma. an] is grammatically 
['ma.ka] 'and' plus [lu. 'ma.an] 'walks'. In (3b) ["ma.ka.lu. 'ma.an] is 
a combination of the prefix [ma.ka-] 'caii, is able' plus [lu. 'ma.an] 
'walks'. In utterance (3c) ["ma.ka.lu.' a.qan] represents [ma] 'at' 
plus [ka-J 'nominalizing prefix' plus the suffixed form [lu. ' a.qan] 
'outside'. Yet all three of these examples appear phonologically to be 
a single word. 

5.2 Stress 

The accentual system consists of two stress levels, primary ['] and 
secondary ["] , both of which axe reewiily piredictable. Stress occurs on 
a syllable peak, whether vowel or consonant, and any syllable which does 
not carrY either, primary or secondeuy stress is said to have zero 
stress. 

Stress has features of both length and prominence (relative loud- 
ness). A stressed syllable is normally both louder and of sli^tly 
longer duration than other syllables in a word. 

The particle class of words have zero stress • All other words re- 
quire primary stress, plus as many secondary stresses as word length 
permits. The rules for stress prediction are described and illustrated 
in the following paragraphs. 

5.2.1 Any one-syllable word, excepting particles, carries primary 
stress. One-syllable peo^icles have zero stress. 

{'boqj 'just now' [du] 'indeed' (emphatic 

particle) 

I'soq] ^but' [giq] 'yet, still' (in- 

completive peurticle) 

5.2.2 Primary stress always occiirs on the penultimate syllable of words 
having two or more syllables. The following examples show how primary 
stress permutes to the peniiltimate position when suffixes are added to a 
word stem. (Prefixes do not cause perturbation of primary stress.) 

^> I'ba.li^kl] 'return' [ba. 'li.k^^n] 'repeat' 

Imag. 'ba.l^kl] 'returning' [ba.li. 'kan.nu] 'you are 

repeating' 



86 The Phonology of Sibutu 

5 •2. 3 Words having two and three syllables cajny only primary stress. 

["m.bal] 'no' [^n-goq] 'mother* 

I'se.heqJ 'companion' Ih^^s. 'sa.la] 'talk' 

[pa. 'n a. am] 'pancake' {qam. 'bu.un] 'type of starch' 

5.2.i+ Words having only four syllables carry a single secondary stress 
on the first syllable, in addition to the primary vord stress. 

["bi.tu. 'qa.nan] 'divorcee' 

["ka.Ja. 'lar^.k^^n] 'scorpion' 

["ka.bwi. 'nal.an] 'agreed' 

["mag.sa. 'b/\n.neQ.] 'telling the truth' 

["ba.li, 'kan.nu] 'you are repeating' 

["b [S . sa. ' la. han] ' persuading ' 

5.2.5 Words having more than four syllables must be considered in terms 
of unprefixed and prefixed forms. 

5.2.5.1 Unprefixed words having more than four syllables carry a single 
secondary stress on the first syllable, in addition to the primary word 
stress. 

["d/gi.da.le. 'qe.t (^n] 'remove the thorns' 

["b\^s.sa.la. 'han.na] 'he is persuading' 
["b^^s. sa.la. han. 'ka. mi] 've are persxiading' 

I"b|^s. sa.la. han. si. 'ge. a] 'they are pers\iading' 



5.2.5.2 When prefixes are added to a word of moire than four syllables, 
perturbation occurs in the secondary stress in certain cases, but the 
primary stress is unaffected. 

5.2,5.2.1 A one-syllable prefix causes fluctuation of the secondary 
stress between the first and second syllables. 

["pa.m\^s.sa. 'la.han]^'|pa."m^^s.sa. 'la. ban] 'instrument for 

speaking' 

['^pag.b^s.sa. 'la.han]AiXpae."b(^s.sa. 'la. han] 'the thing 

spoken about' 

["ta.ba.li.'kan.nu]Nlta.''b».li.'k«i.au] 'you can repeat' 



The Phonology of Sibutu 8? 



5.2. 5-2.2 A tvo syllable prefix carries obligatory secondary stress on 
its first syllable, and any secondary stress on the vord stem 

remains lanchanged. 

["ma.ka.b^^s. 'sa.la] 'able to talk' 

["pi.na."b^^s. sa. 'la.hanj'^'{"ni .pa."b^^s. sa. 'la.han] 'to be 

persuaded' 

["pi.nag."bi^s.sa. 'la.han]'X'|"ni.pag."b|^s.sa. 'la.han] 'the thing 

being spoken 
about ' 

5.2.5.2.3 Combinations of tvo one-syllable prefixes are stressed and 
function as if they were one tvo-sy liable prefix. 

["ka.pag.b(^s. 'sa.la] 'able to talk to each other' 

I"ta.pag."b|^s.sa. ' la.han] 'the thing able to be spoken about' 
["mag.pa.b^^s. 'sa.la] 'allowing to speak' 

5.2. 5.2. U In combinations of two, two-syllable prefixes, each prefix 
carries stress on the first syllable and the secondary stress on the 
word stem disappears. 

["ma.ka, "pag. pa. bi^s.sa. ' la.han] 'able to cause persuasion' 

5 ,2. 5. 2. 5 In combinations of two-syllable pl\is one-syllable prefixes, 
one secondary stress occvirs initially on the two-syllable prefix and 
another fluctuates between the one-syllable prefix and the word-stem 
initial syllable. 

["ma.ka.pag."b\^s.sa. 'la.han]'V'["ma.ka."pag.b\^s.sa. 'la.han] 'able 

to persuade them' 

["ta.pag.pa."b|^s.sa.la. 'han.bi]'V'["ta.pag."pa.b^s.sa.la. 'han.bi] 

'yo^ (pl) Q^^ able to make them 
persuade s.o.' 

5.2.6 When reduplicated word stems occur, each of the reduplicated 
portions are stressed in accordance with the forgoing rules, except that 
(1) the stress on the first word stem, which voiold normally be primaiy 
stress, is replaced by secondary stress, and (2) word-initial stress 
does not fluctuate between the first two syllables as described in 
5.2.5.2.1. 

["'ha.^an. 'ha.^an] 'breezy, fresh' 
[mag. "ha. ^an . ' ha. ^an] ' airing' 
[pa."tDg.g5l.t3g. 'go.lun] 'leave it longer' 



88 The Phonology of Sihutu 



^mag.hi^s/'sa.la.b^^s. 'sa.la 3 'talking and talking 
^"pi.na."'b\^s. sa.b^^s. 'sa.han^ 'being scolded by 



I 
I 



5 •2. 7 Personal names and kinship terms used as vocatives have a 
lengthened final syllable vith the usiial accompanying stress shift. 

[si 'putl.lij 'girl's name [potl.li.i] 'name as 

vocative' 

I'n.goq] 'mother' [n.'go.oq] 'oh, mother' 



FOOTNOTES 

Other languages in the Southern Sama group include Tandubas, 
Simunul, Manuk Mankaw^ Pahut, Sempoma (Sabah) and various subdialects 
on Sibutu and Simunul Islands. There are no doubt additional languages 
belonging to this family in both Sabah, Malaysia, and Kalimantan, In- 
donesia, but a full investigation has not yet been conducted. 

2 
Pallesen, A. Kemp, "Culture Contact and Language Convergence," 

Ph. D. Dissertation, University of California at Berkeley, 1977. 

3 
The figures are based on the 1975 integrated census of population 

for Tawi-Tavi province, corrected for an estimated nvimber of Tausug 

speakers. 

The data for this study were collected between 1976 and 1978 in 
the barrio (township) of Sibutu Proper, on the island of Sibutu, in the 
province of Tawi-Tavi » undex the auspices of the Summer Institute of 
Ling\iistics. The principal language assistant was Wilhelmina Mandaling, 
a female 20 years of age. Some work was also conducted with the same 
assistant in the city of Zamboanga. 

Pallesen, Kenp and Anne. "Phonemes of Samal of Sulu", an un- 
published paper in the files of the Summer Institute of Linguistics, 
Philippine Branch, Manila. No date. 



The Phonology of Sibutu 89 

This interpretation requires that the stop phonemes be understood 
as functioning in syllable peaks, a most unusual phenomenon among the 
world's languages. In addition to the synchronic data given in the text, 
there is also diachronic evidence supporting this interpretation • Com- 
parative studies (Pallesen, fn.2) show that forms with stem-initial 
homorganic and geminate consonant clusters are reflexes of Proto-Sama 
forms in which a schwa */9/ original .ly preceded the consonant cluster. 
Thus: I'p.paq]<*['3p.paq], pt.tuq]<*I 'Bt.tuqJ ,[ 'k.kaq]<* [ 'Sk.kaq] , 
etc. The proto-schwa in Sibutu Sama lost its contrastive status and 
disappesLred stem- initially, leaving the homorganic clusters in stem- 
initial position. Now, most of these forms comprise only two syllables, 
and carry stress on the pen\ilt, both in the proto-form and in the modem 
form. This stress did not disappear along with the schwa, but has been 
retained, being carried by the first member of the initial consonant 
cluster. Pallesen also notes for Siasa Sama the disappearance (phonetic- 
ally) of /9/ in stem-initial position, but the retention of the stress. 
The process is similar for both Sibutu and Siasi Sama, except that in 
the case of the former, phonemic status of schwa is lost, while for the 
latter it is retained. In Siasa Sama the schwa is realized when a con- 
sonant-final prefix is added to the stem, but this does not occur in 
Sibutu Sama. 

This statement is a convenient generalization. The actual 
situation is somewhat more complex, in that the released-unreleased 
variation appears to be conditioned by the time lapse between the end of 
one word and the beginning of the next. If the timing is very short, 
the released phone will occur. If the timing is relatively long, the 
unreleased phone will occur. This relative timing, of course, varies^ 
with each individual speaker and cannot be predetermined. Hence, it is 
not unrealistic to speak here of free variation. 

It is posited that each of the vowel phonemes except /a/ has an 
unstressed allophone occurring in closed syllables, namely, [J , [^]> 
[e] and [:>]. While these phones are readily distinguished in the 
environment preceding geminate and homorganic consonant cloisters, there 
are problems in their perception in unstressed closed syllables (C — ^C)* 
[J and [u] are the only two which are clearly perceptible in this 
environment, [e] and [:>] are so close to [e] and [o] (respectively) 
that distinction is almost totally obscured. However, the adoption of 
this inteipretation seems reasonable in view of the perceptible dis- 
tinctive differences between [i] , [(\ and between [u] , [u] which 
establishes a pattern that miy be extended to [e] , [e] and [o] , [d] on 
the basis of phonetic symmetiy. With regard to /a/, however, there is 
no question that the allophone [A] does not occur in the unstressed 
C ^C environment. 

^The variation [Aj-vfaJ appears to be spreading to words in which 
the vowel following the homorganic or geminate cluster is /i/, although 



90 The Phonology of Sibutu 

the process is not yet generalized. Examples are: 

I'JAn.tiqJ'vpjan.tiqJ 'pretty' 

TpAp.JiJ'vI'pan.jiJ 'flag' 

I 'pAn. di]'^]; 'pan. di] 'bathe' 



Zero stress is a technical term standing for the absence of 
primary and secondaiy stress. However, even when the primary and 
secondary stress are abaftnt, some form of stress is obviously still 
present, inasmuch as no syllable can be uttered vithout both relative 
loudness and length. The norm that occxirs vithout specieO. stress 
accentuation is here referred to as zero stress. 



STUDIES IN PHILIPPINE LINGUISTICS 



Edited by 
Casilda Edrial-Luzares 

Austin Hale 



Managing Editor 
Marjorie Cook 



VOLUME 3 1979 NUMBER 2 

LINGUISTIC SOCIETY OF THE PHILIPPINES 
SUMMER INSTITUTE OF LINGUISTICS