Skip to main content

Full text of "Some facts in partial justification of the so-called dogma of discipline"

See other formats


EXCHANGE 


ft. 


MAR  21 


UNIVERSITY   OF    ILLINOIS    BULLETIN 

Vol.  VII.  OCTOBER  17,  1909  No.  7 

[Entered  Feb.  14,  1902,  at  Urbana,  111.,  as  second  class  matter  under  Act  of  Congress  July  16, 18941 


UNIVERSITY  OF  ILLINOIS 

SCHOOL  OF  EDUCATION 

BULLETIN  NO.  2 

SOME  FACTS  IN  PARTIAL  JUSTIFICATION  OF  THE 
SO-CALLED  DOGMA  OF  DISCIPLINE 

BY 

STEPHEN  S.  COLVIN 
Professor  of  Psychology,  University  of  Illinois 


URBANA-CHAMPAIGN,  ILLINOIS 

PUBLISHED  BY  THE  UNIVERSITY 

f  UNIVE~~;TY  j 


UNIVERSITY  OF  ILLINOIS 

SCHOOL  OF  EDUCATION 

BULLETIN  NO.  2 

SOME  FACTS  IN  PARTIAL  JUSTIFICATION  OF  THE 
SO-CALLED  DOGMA  OF  DISCIPLINE 


BY 


STEPHEN  S.  COLVIN 
Professor  of  Psychology,  University  of  Illinois 


URBANA-CHAMPAIGN,  ILLINOIS 

PUBLISHED  BY  THE  UNIVERSITY 


SOME  FACTS  IN  PARTIAL  JUSTIFICATION  OF  THE 
SO-CALLED  DOGMA  OF  FORMAL  DISCIPLINE 

I.     The  Statement  of  the  Problem 5 

II.  Experimental  Evidence  Relating  to  the  Problem  .         .    .  8 

III.  The  Possibility  of  Forming  a  Generalized  Habit        .         .  17 

IY.  Rules  for  Securing  Transfer        ......  24 

V.     The  Superior  Disciplinary   Value  of  Pure  as  Compared 

with  Applied  Science 26 


INTEODUCTOEY  STATEMENT 

This  paper  substantially  as  printed  was  read  before  the  Illi- 
nois School  Masters'  Club  at  Peoria,  Oct.  8,  1909.  On  the  follow- 
ing morning  the  question  of  formal  discipline  was  discussed,  Pres- 
ident David  Felmley  of  the  Illinois  State  Normal  University, 
leading.  There  seemed  to  be  a  substantial  agreement  that  trans- 
fer of  training  was  possible  to  a  greater  or  less  degree,  and 
that  it  was  best  accomplished  by  making  the  habit  set  up  a 
conscious  end  of  action.  President  Felmley  disagreed  with  the 
speaker  of  the  previous  evening  chiefly  on  two  points :  ( 1 )  That 
a  "generalized"  habit  was  possible  and  (2)  That  pure  science  had 
a  superior  value  to  applied  science.  In  the  light  of  President 
Felmley's  discussion  these  two  topics  in  the  paper  have  been 
slightly  amplified.  Otherwise  the  paper  is  printed  as  read. 

University  of  Illinois,  Nov.  15,  1909. 


I.  THE  STATEMENT  OF  THE  PROBLEM. 

In  the  early  years  of  the  nineteenth  century  Gall  and  his  pu- 
pil Spurzheim  gave  to  the  world  iheAnatomie  et  physiologic  dm 
systeme  nerveux,  in  which  is  to  be  found  a  detailed  exposition  of 
Gall's  System  of  Phrenology.  According  to  this  system  the  brain 
is  supposed  to  contain  more  than  thirty  separate  and  individual  or- 
gans which  are  the  seat  of  the  most  complex  psychic  capacities, 
or  internal  senses,  such  as  combativeness,  the  fear-of-God,  a  sense- 
of-fact,  the  impluse-of-self-preservation,  philoprogenitiveness, 
and  the  sense-of-language.  It  was  only  an  incident  to  this  system 
that  the  locality  of  these  internal  senses  was  found  on  the  surface 
of  the  brain,  and  that  the  external  evidence  for  them  existed  in 
certain  prominences  on  the  skull.  The  interest  in  this  now  biz- 
arre theory  lies  as  far  as  the  discussion  of  this  evening  is  con- 
cerned, in  the  fact  that  here  we  find  in  a  most  pronounced  form 
two  basal  assumptions,  one  of  which  has  served  as  a  convenient 
vehicle  for  the  justification  of  the  dogma  of  formal  discipline, 
while  the  other  lies,  in  part  at  least,  at  the  basis  of  the  theories 
of  those  who  in  their  reaction  against  this  dogma  have  gone  in 
the  other  direction  to  extremes  which  seem  equally  absurd  and 
incapable  of  justification.  The  first  of  these  assumptions  is  that 
the  mind  is  composed  of  a  number  of  separate  faculties,  and  from 
this  it  readily  followed  that  these  can  be  educated  in  their  entirety 
and  made  to  serve  in  the  various  situations  of  life  equally  well  for 
all  purposes.  The  second  assumption  has  looked  upon  the  va- 
rious activities  of  the  nervous  system,  particularly  of  the  cortex, 
as  highly  specialized  and  definitely  localized,  and  has  viewed  the 
corresponding  psychic  functions  as  something  quite  discrete  and 
separate.  Carried  to  its  logical  extreme  it  would  seem  to 
imply  that,  for  example,  there  is  not  only  a  cortical  centre  for 
language,  but  a  distinct  area  for  nouns,  or  for  verbs,  or  what 
not;  not  only  a  cortical  centre  for  vision,  but  a  definite  area  for 
color;  then  why  not  for  all  the  thirty-two  thousand  color  quali- 
ties which  the  eye  can  sense?  There  seems  to  be  no  limit  to  the 
multiplication  of  centres  which  can  result  from  such  a  theory, 
and  it  surely  serves  as  a  firm  foundation  for  the  doctrine  that 
there  is  no  education  in  general,  and  that  the  best  we  can  do  is  to 
train  the  individual  to  interpret  a  certain  number  of  definite 
sense  stimuli  and  to  respond  to  a  limited  number  of  concrete  situ- 
ations in  the  same  old  way.  The  faculty  psychology  assumed  a 


6  UNIVERSITY  OF  ILLINOIS 

number  of  fabulous  entities  which  worked  out  the  destinies  of  the 
individual,  while  the  doctrine  of  absolute  localization  of  nervous 
function  has  made  the  brain  a  machine  of  relatively  unrelated 
parts  and  has  created  a  doctrine  of  psychic  atomism  which  is  as 
untrue  as  it  is  impossible  of  practical  application. 

The  faculty  psychology  of  the  last  century  is  long  since  dead, 
and  its  resting  place  has  almost  been  forgotten  by  the  scientist  of 
today ;  its  ghost,  however,  stalks  abroad  among  the  masses,  and  its 
spirit  still  lives  in  the  pedagogical  theories  of  many  an  uncritical 
thinker.  From  this  faculty  psychology  it  is  no  far  cry  to  the 
dogma  of  formal  discipline  in  all  its  purity.  The  assumptions  it 
contains  are  well  expressed  by  a  clergyman  quoted  by  Prof.  James 
m  the  first  volume  of  his  larger  Psychology :  "As  for  my  memory'* 
writes  the  clergyman,  "it  has  improved  year  by  year ....  like  a 
gymnast's  muscle".  This  is  a  favorite  comparison,  the  likening 
of  memory,  or  attention  or  any  other  supposed  psychic  faculty  t<f 
a  muscle  that  can  be  developed  for  any  use  by  any  kind  of  exer- 
cise, and  that  is  made  equally  strong  by  rowing,  or  boxing,  or 
chopping  wood,  provided  that  the  exercise  is  vigorous  enough; 
and  having  been  made  strong  by  one  exercise,  can  be  used  equally 
well  for  all  activities.  No  less  a  scientist  than  Helmholtz  is  quoted 
by  Coover  and  Angell1  as  valuing  particularly  certain  studies  as  a 
means  of  intellectual  training  since  these  studies  taxed  "equally 
all  the  intellectual  powers".  Here  we  have  the  doctrine  of  form- 
al discipline  and  its  pedagogical  consequence  expressed  definitely 
and  clearly.  The  implication  of  Helmholtz  assumption  seems 
to  be  that  there  are  definite  mental  powers  and  that  these  powers 
can  be  developed  in  all  directions  by  certain  well  chosen  studies. 

Bagley  in  his  Educative  Process2  puts  the  matter  concretely 
in  this  way;  "Certain  subjects  of  the  curriculum,  if  properly  pur- 
sued, were  believed  to  develop  what  might  be  termed  'generalized' 
habits.  For  example,  a  pupil  may  acquire  a  specific  habit  of  pro- 
ducing neat  papers  in  arithmetic.  The  doctrine  of  formal  disci- 
pline assumes  that  if  this  habit  is  once  thoroughly  established,  it 
will  function  equally  well  in  connection  with  language  and  draw- 
ing; that,  functioning  successfully  here,  it  cannot  fail  to  insure 
neatness  of  person  and  attire  and  that  the  habit  of  neatness  thus 
ingrained  upon  the  pupil  will  surely  be  carried  into  mature 
years." 

1 American  Journal  o/  Psychology,  Vol.  XVIII,  pages  328-340  (1907). 
'Chapter  XIII,  page  203. 


THE  DOGMA  OF  FORMAL  DISCIPLINE  7 

Thus  it  is  assumed  that  there  is  a  general  faculty  or  habit  of 
neatness,  and  that  this  when  trained  by  one  set  of  exercises,  will 
be  serviceable  in  all  the  specific  situations  in  life  where  neatness 
may  be  employed.  Stated  thus  the  dogma  of  formal  discipline  is 
absolutely  untenable.  There  is  no  general  faculty  of  neatness, 
nor  of  any  mental  capacity,  and  if  there  were  such  an  entity, 
training  it  to  function  in  one  direction  would  not  mean  that  it 
was  trained  equally  well  to  function  in  all  directions.  If  there 
was  nothing  more  to  the  doctrine  of  formal  discipline  than  the 
old  faculty  psychology,  or  the  thought  that  training  in  one  direc- 
tion can  be  transferred  equally  well  in  all  directions,  I  should 
not  attempt  even  a  partial  justification  of  it  here.  It  is  quite 
obvious  and  beyond  argument  that  training  in  mathematical 
reasoning  does  not  necessarily  mean  ability  to  reason  equally 
well  in  the  affairs  of  every  day  life ;  it  seems  certain  that  if  I  wish 
to  increase  my  ability  to  discriminate  between  shades  of  gray,  the 
best  training  is  to  attempt  such  discriminations  and  not,  for  ex- 
ample, to  practice  discriminating  between  intensities  of  sound. 
It  is  a  vastly  different  matter  to  affirm,  however,  that  mathemat- 
ical reasoning  has  no  effect  on  other  rational  processes,  and  that 
as  far  as  distinguishing  shades  of  gray  is  concerned  it  is  quite  in- 
different whether  the  person  has  had  previous  training  in  sensory 
discrimination  in  other  fields.  To  affirm  that  when  the  mind  is 
trained  in  one  direction  it  is  first  of  all  trained  in  that  direction 
and  not  in  some  other  is  one  thing;  to  affirm,  however,  that  the 
training  in  one  direction  has  no  influence  in  other  direction  is 
quite  a  different  affair.  No  one,  I  think,  would  be  quite  so  rash 
as  to  make  the  latter  assertion;  but  many  would  believe  that 
such  a  transfer  of  training  is  in  most  instances  slight,  and  in  some 
cases  such  a  transfer  is  not  probable,  even  in  the  slightest  degree. 
Some  seem  to  assume  that  about  all  that  education  can  hope  to 
do  is  to  give  to  the  pupil  certain  facts  in  a  limited  department  of 
knowledge,  and  to  habituate  him  to  definite  reactions  in  a  cir- 
cumscribed field  of  human  activity.  They  seem  to  despair  of  any 
education  in  general  that  amounts  to  much.  Having,  however, 
admitted  the  possibility  of  transfer  from  one  field  to  another,never 
mind  how  little,  they  are  incapable  of  determining  a  priori  how 
great  this  transfer  may  be  and  what  general  effects  it  may  have. 
Such  persons  may  try  to  bring  some  definiteness  into  their  con- 
ceptions as  to  the  extent  of  this  transfer,  by  saying  that  such  a 
transfer  can  take  place  only  where  there  is  a  similar  situation, 
and  where  like  elements  are  involved ;  but  it  must  be  remembered 


8  UNIVERSITY  OF  ILLINOIS 

that  similarity  and  likeness  are  not  primarily  objective  categories, 
but  that  they  are  constituted  by  the  mind  of  the  person  who  finds 
such  similarity  or  likeness,  and  that  it  is  never  certain  before- 
hand just  where  this  similarity  and  likeness  is  to  be  found. 

Such  a  contention  can  be  determined  only  by  actual  tests 
either  in  life  itself,  or  in  the  psychological  experiment.  Fortu- 
nately we  have  had  in  the  last  few  years  a  considerable  number 
of  such  experiments  reported,  and  the  light  which  they  throw  on 
the  whole  question  of  formal  discipline  is  more  illuminating  than 
that  which  can  be  secured  from  a  discussion  of  a  priori  assump- 
tions, or  'half -baked'  psychological  theories.  I  therefore  turn  to 
them  and  call  the  most  important  of  them  briefly  to  your  atten- 
tion. 

II.     EXPERIMENTAL  EVIDENCE  RELATING  TO  THE 

PROBLEM. 

The  two  pieces  of  experimental  evidence  which  have  probably 
had  the  most  influence  in  descrediting  the  doctrine  of  formal  dis- 
cipline and  in  over-emphasizing  the  opposite  doctrine  are  those 
of  Prof.  James  of  two  decades  ago,  and  the  more  recent  series  in- 
spired by  Prof.  Thorndike  of  Columbia  University.  James  in  his 
chapter  on  Memory  in  the  larger  Psychology3  says:  All  improve- 
ment of  memory  consists,  then,  in  the  improvement  of  one's  habit- 
ual methods  of  recording  facts.  By  this  he  means  to  deny  that 
there  is  any  improvement  in  memory-power  as  such,  the  improve- 
ment being  solely  due  to  the  method  or  the  technique  of  memoriz- 
ing. He  then  gives  in  a  footnote  a  description  of  certain  tests 
carried  on  by  himself  in  support  of  his  assertion.  Since  these 
tests  are  the  first  of  a  considerable  number  of  later  experiments 
conducted  in  a  similar  way,  I  will  venture  here  to  give  a  brief 
description  of  their  general  nature.  Like  those  that  have  been 
undertaken  later  by  other  investigators,  the  essential  technique 
of  these  earliest  experiments  consisted  in  giving  the  subject  of  the 
test  some  material  to  learn,  thereby  determining  his  capacity 
for  learning  as  measured  by  a  certain  standard.  The  material  for 
this  first  learning  constitutes  what  is  termed  the  "test  series" ;  it 
is  followed  by  a  period  of  practice  in  learning  some  other  kind  of 
material  and  this  second  material  is  known  as  the  "practice  se- 
ries"; this  practice  series  is  then  followed  by  another  learning  of 
the  material  of  the  test  series,  and  the  improvement  or  lack  of 

•Vol.  1,  page  667. 


THE  DOGMA  OF  FORMAL  DISCIPLINE  9 


improvement  over  the  first  trials  gives  a  measure  of  the  effect  of 
the  practice  series  on  the  ability  to  learn. 

To  take  a  concrete  case,  James  tested  himself  by  learning  on 
eight  successive  days  150  lines  of  Victor  Hugo's  'Satyr'.  He 
says  :  "The  total  number  of  minutes  required  for  this  was  131  5-6 
—  it  should  be  said  that  I  had  learned  nothing  by  heart  for  many 
years.  I  then,  working  for  twenty-odd  minutes  daily,  learned  the 
entire  first  book  of  Paradise  Lost,  occupying  38  consecutive  days 
in  the  process".  He  then  went  back  to  the  learning  of  Victor 
Hugo  and  found  that  while  before  the  training  he  had  learned  at 
the  rate  of  one  line  in  50  seconds,  after  the  training  he  learned 
at  the  slower  rate  of  one  line  in  57  seconds.  James  added  that 
during  the  second  learning  series  he  was  perceptibly  fagged  with 
other  work,  which  of  course,  invalidated  the  entire  experiment. 

Other  persons  under  Professor  James7  direction  carried  on 
similar  experiments  under  somewhat  better  conditions  and  the  re- 
sults showed  a  slight  positive  effect  of  the  training. 

These  experiments  are  today  chiefly  of  historical  interest. 
They  were  not  carried  on  under  the  strictest  experimental  condi- 
tions and  are  valuable  mainly  as  pioneer  investigations  in  the 
field.  They  are  important  also  because  their  conclusions  were 
given  to  the  world  with  the  tremendous  authority  that  the  ipse 
dixit  of  their  author  has  always  carried.  Thus  they  have  done 
much  to  reform  the  entire  notion  of  the  possibilities  of  memory 
training  and  of  training  in  general. 

The  second  set  of  experiments  which  I  have  mentioned  above 
as  having  had  an  important  pedagogical  consequence  are  those  of 
Thorndike  and  Woodworth4  reported  under  the  title,  The  Influ- 
ence of  Improvement  in  One  Mental  Function  upon  the  Efficien- 
cy of  Other  Functions.  Tests  were  conducted  to  determine  the 
influence  of  the  training  in  the  estimation  of  magnitudes  on  the 
ability  to  estimate  magnitudes  of  the  same  general  sort;  the  influ- 
ence of  training  in  estimating  weights,  on  the  ability  to  estimate 
the  weight  of  miscellaneous  objects  of  similar  weight  ;  and  the  in- 
fluence of  the  practice  in  marking  words  containing  certain  letters 
on  the  marking  of  words  containing  other  letters,  misspelled 
words,  et  cetera. 

In  these  varied  tests  most  of  the  subjects  showed  some  im- 

provement when  tested  after  the  practice  series.     Thorndike's 

general  conclusion  is  that  while  there  is  some  transfer  it  is  not 

due  to  any  "mysterious  transfer  of  practice,  to  an  unanalyzable 

'Psychological  Review,  Vol.  VIII,  pages  247-261;   348-395;   553-564. 


10  UNIVERSITY  OF  ILLINOIS 

property  of  mental  functions",  but  rather  to  a  transfer  of  identi- 
cal elements  from  the  practice  series  to  the  final  test  series.  This 
transfer  on  the  whole  does  not  seem  to  be  great,  and  its  spread  is 
limited  largely  to  activities  that  closely  resemble  one  another. 
Thorndike's  experiments  have  been  criticised  as  giving  results  in 
part  at  varience  with  his  conclusions,  and  as  lacking  entirely  in 
introspective  analysis,  making  it  difficult  to  interpret  the  true 
significance  of  the  numerical  results. 

Another  experimental  study  which  seems  to  point  in  part  to 
the  same  general  conclusions  as  those  of  Thorndike  and  Wood- 
worth  was  conducted  also  at  Columbia  University  by  Bair5.  This 
experimenter  made  an  extensive  investigation  and  analysis  of  the 
practice  curve.  The  writer,  however,  concludes  that  "any  bit  of 
special  training  also  helps  us  to  receive  training  in  general.  Any 
training  helps  us  to  find  ourselves.  It  gives  us  a  method  of  orien- 
tation which  leaves  us  in  our  reactions  not  entirely  at  the  mercy 
of  chance  even  in  unfamiliar  situations.  The  experience  which 
we  get  from  special  training  gives  us  a  general  power  to  meet  an 
entirely  new  situation  with  a  more  favorable  response  than  had 
we  not  had  this  special  training". 

Among  the  earlier  experiments,  the  transfer  of  training  from 
one  specific  set  of  reactions  to  another  was  investigated  under 
the  direction  of  Scripture  in  the  psychological  laboratory  at  Yale 
University6.  The  first  series  of  experiments  considered  the  in- 
crease of  muscular  steadiness  through  practice  in  inserting  a 
needle  in  a  very  small  hole,  and  the  transfer  of  this  increase  to  the 
corresponding  muscles  of  the  opposite  half  of  the  body.  The  left 
hand  was  tested  first  and  showed  50  per  cent  of  correct  trials,  but 
after  practice  with  the  right  hand  for  ten  days  the  left  hand 
showed  76  per  cent  of  successes.  Scripture  explains  these  results 
as  due  primarily  to  a  training  of  the  attention  rather  than  to  any 
carrying  over  of  skill  in  adjustment.  Experiments  on  the  in- 
crease of  muscular  power  after  practice  showed  "  a  steady  in- 
crease in  the  muscular  power  of  the  right  hand  due  to  practice, 
and  also  an  increase  in  the  power  of  the  left  hand  due  to  what 
might  be  called  'indirect  practice'  ". 

BColumbia  University,  Contributions  to  Philosophy,  Psychology  and  Educa- 
tion, Vol.  IX  (1902). 

"On  the  education  of  muscular  control  and  power,  E.  W.  Scripture,  T.  L. 
Smith,  and  Emily  M.  Brown,  Studies  from  the  Yale  Psychological  Laboratory. 
Vol.  II,  pages  105-114,  (1894).  Also  Researches  in  cross-education  by  Walter  W. 
Davis;  ibid,  Vol.  VI,  pages  6-50  (1898)  and  VIII,  pages  64-109  (1900). 


THE  DOGMA  OF  FORMAL  DISCIPLINE  11 

Further  experiments  conducted  by  Davis  under  Scripture's 
direction  on  the  rapidity  of  tapping  a  telegraph  key  showed  im- 
provement through  practice  not  only  for  the  part  of  the  -body 
practiced  but  for  other  members  as  well.  Experiments  in 
strength  of  voluntary  effort  in  lifting  dumbells  showed  a  trans- 
ferrence  of  the  effects  of  practice  from  the  right  to  the  left  arm 
and  in  muscular  development  and  endurance.  Experiments  in 
lunging  at  a  target  with  a  fencer's  foil  showed  that  practice  with 
the  right  hand  affected  the  left  hand  positively.  According  to 
Davis  the  results  of  the  experiments  showed  not  only  that  effects 
of  exercise  may  be  transferred,  but  also  that  "will  power  and  at- 
tention are  educated  by  physical  training  and  that  when  devel- 
oped by  any  special  act  they  are  developed  for  all  other  acts". 

Later  Judd7  carried  on  a  series  of  experiments  on  the  effect 
of  practice  without  knowledge  of  results. 

The  person  tested  was  required  to  judge  the  length  of  certain 
lines  and  he  was  seated  in  such  a  position  that  his  right  hand 
and  arm  were  entirely  hidden  from  view  by  a  large  screen.  What- 
ever he  did  with  his  right  hand  was,  therefore,  unseen  by  him. 
"On  the  left  side  of  the  screen  and  in  full  view,  nine  different  lines 
were  shown  in  succession,  and  he  was  required  to  place  a  pencil 
held  in  the  unseen  right  hand  in  the  direction  indicated  by  the 
several  lines  seen  before  him".  After  this  the  reactor  was  given 
"fuller  visual  experience"  with  one  line  and  an  improvement  at 
once  took  place  in  regard  to  this  line.  This  improvement  was 
found  to  be  transferred  by  later  test  to  the  other  lines,  but  in 
some  instances  this  transfer  was  negative.  The  lines  that  in  the 
original  series  had  shown  an  error  similar  to  that  of  the  line  with 
which  fuller  visual  experience  had  been  obtained,  showed  a  posi- 
tive improvement  in  the  test  series,  those  in  which  the  error  had 
been  in  an  oppisite  direction  grew  worse.  In  both  cases,  how- 
ver,  there  were  clear  evidences  of  a  transfer  effect,  but  in  the 
second  case  the  transfer  was  negative. 

In  a  second  series  of  tests,  geometrical  figures  were  compared. 
Because  of  an  illusion,  one  of  these  was  over  estimated,  another 
underestimated.  During  the  experiment  one  observer  was  kept 
in  ignorance  of  the  results,  while  the  other  was  funy  informed. 
Then  the  figures  were  reversed  and  a  second  series  of  tests  were 
begun.  In  this  the  observer  who  knew  the  effect  of  practice  ad- 
7These  experiments  are  reported  in  the  Psychological  Review,  Vol.  IX,  pages 
27-30  (1902);  also  in  a  symposium  on  formal  discipline  in  the  Educational  Re^ 
view,  June  1908. 


12  UNIVERSITY  OF  ILLINOIS 

justed  himself  to  the  new  conditions.  The  other  observer  who  did 
not  know  the  effects  showed  a  greater  error  than  at  any  time,  and 
was  unable  to  improve,  because,  as  Judd  believes,  the  habit  had 
now  became  so  firmly  fixed  that  training  could  no  longer  modify 
the  reaction. 

A  similar  test  was  later  carried  on  with  school  children,  who 
were  required  to  hit  a  target  placed  under  water.  This  was  diffi- 
cult because  of  the  deflection  of  the  light  through  refraction.  In 
the  test  one  group  was  instructed  in  the  nature  of  refraction,  while 
the  others  were  kept  in  ignorance.  The  boys  in  this  test  who  had 
been  instructed  did  no  better  than  the  others,  but  in  a  second  test 
in  which  the  depth  of  the  target  in  the  water  was  reduced  from 
twelve  inches  to  four,  the  boys  who  had  the  theory  fitted  them- 
selves quickly  to  the  new  conditions,  while  with  the  other  group 
the  errors  were  large  and  persistent.  These  experiments  clearly 
show  the  value  of  a  knowledge  of  conditions  in  connection  with 
the  transfer  of  training. 

A  somewhat  analogous  fact  is  brought  out  by  Buediger  in  a 
test  reported  in  the  Educational  Review8  for  November,  1908. 
Kuediger's  experiment  was  suggested  by  Bagley's  results9  with 
children  tested  at  the  Montana  State  Normal  College.  Bagley 
attempted  "to  determine  whether  the  habit  of  producing  neat 
papers  in  arithmetic  will  function  with  reference  to  neat  written 
work  in  other  studies".  He  states  that  "the  results  are  almost 
startling  in  their  failure  to  show  the  slightest  improvement  in 
language  and  spelling  papers,  although  the  improvement  in  the 
arithmetic  papers  was  noticeable  from  the  first."  Ruediger's  tests 
were  carried  on  for  eight  weeks  in  three  different  schools,  all  of 
the  seventh  grade.  He  found  that  when  the  emphasis  on  neatness 
in  one  subject  was  accompanied  by  talks  to  pupils  on  neatness, 
so  that  the  habit  was  raised  to  clear  consciousness,  ^decided  im- 
provement was  shown  in  subjects  no  more  closely  related  than 
geography,  arithmetic,  grammar,  and  history.  In  other  words, 
the  habit  was  constructed  into  an  ideal  and  a  transfer  was  thus 
made  which  seemed  entirely  lacking  when  the  whole  procedure 
was  on  the  level  of  the  subconscious.  This  matter  will  be  brought 
up  again  in  a  later  part  of  the  discussion. 

Another  important  series  of  experiments  on  transfer  was 
conducted  a  few  years  ago  by  Coover  and  Angell10  on  the  general 

"Improvement  of  mental  functions  through  ideals! 

'The  Educative  Process,  Chap.  XIII,  p.  208. 

™Americ<m  Joivrnal  of  Psychology,  Vol.  XVIII,  pages  328-340   (1907). 


THE  DOGMA  OF  FORMAL  DISCIPLINE  13 

practice  effects  of  special  exercises.  In  one  experiment  the  train- 
ing series  consisted  in  the  discrimination  of  sound  intensities,  the 
test  series  in  discriminating  shades  of  gray.  A  clear  transfer  of 
the  practice  effect  was  shown,  as  was  also  the  case  in  a  lesser  de- 
gree in  a  second  test  in  which  the  training  series  consisted  in 
sorting  cards  and  the  test  series  in  typewriter  reactions.  The  au- 
thors conclude  from  the  objective  results  and  the  introspections 
of  the  subjcts  that  the  improvement  consists  in  "divesting  the 
essential  process  of  the  unessential  factors.  There  is  a  greater 
habituation  and  more  economical  adaption  of  *  attention." 

Franker11  recently  carried  on  a  series  of  experiments  on  the 
transference  of  training  in  memory  in  the  psychological  labora- 
tory of  the  State  University  of  Iowa.  The  same  general  methods 
in  the  use  of  training  and  practice  series  were  employed  as  in  the 
previous  experiments  cited. 

The  training  series  consisted  in  memorizing  the  order  of  four 
tones.  The  test  series  were  eight  in  number  as  follows:  (1) 
memory  for  poetry;  (2)  memory  for  the  order  of  four  shades  of 
gray;  (3)  memory  for  the  order  of  nine  tones;  (4)  memory  for 
the  order  of  nine  shades  of  gray;  (5)  memory  for  the  order  of 
four  tones;  (6)  memory  for  the  order  of  nine  geometrical  figures; 
(7)  memory  for  the  order  of  nine  numbers;  (8)  memory  for  the 
extent  of  arm  movement.  Clear  indication  of  transfer  was  found, 
generally  more  marked  in  those  cases  where  the  test  series  and 
the  practice  series  were  similar,  although  it  sometimes  happened 
as  in  Thorndike's  experiments  that  improvement  was  not  noted  in 
some  cases  where  there  was  great  similarity  between  the  practice 
and  test  series;  indeed,  the  practice  seemed  to  have  a  negative  ef- 
fect. The  results  of  the  experiment  did  not,  however,  support 
Thorndike's  contention  that  "improvement  in  any  single  mental 
function  rarely  brings  about  equal  improvement  in  any  other 
function,  no  matter  how  similar".  In  many  cases  Franker  found 
that  improvement  was  absolutely  greater  in  amount  in  the  test 
than  in  the  training  series.  During  the  experiment  the  intro- 
spections of  the  observers  were  carefully  recorded  and  they  indi- 
cate that  mental  imagery  and  properly  controlled  attention  have 
much  to  do  with  the  transfer. 

Another  investigation  of  some  importance  in  clearly  in- 
dicating the  existence  of  transfer  is  that  of  Winch12  who  gave  his 

uThe  Psychological  Review  Monograph  Supplements,  Vol.  IX,  No.  2,  pages 
56-102. 

^British  Journal  of  Psychology,  Vol.  II,  p.  284. 


14  UNIVERSITY  OF  ILLINOIS 

observers  as  a  test  series,  the  learning  of  selections  from  an  his- 
torical reader  and  as  a  training  series  the  committing  of  poetry. 
More  than  one  hundred  children  were  tested.  The  investigator 
concludes  that  "improvement  gained  by  practice  in  memorizing 
one  subject  of  instruction,  is  transferred  to  the  memory  work  in 
other  subjects  whose  nature  is  certainly  diverse  from  that  in 
which  the  improvement  was  gained".  Rote  memory  can  certainly 
be  improved. 

Among  minor  investigations  and  observations  bearing  more 
or  less  directly  on  the  problem  of  the  evening's  discussion  may  be 
mentioned  Volkmann's13  tests  of  a  half  century  ago  on  the  influ- 
ence of  practice  on  spatial  discrimination.  He  investigated  the 
fineness  of  space  discrimination  on  the  skin  by  means  of  the  Weber 
compass  and  found  that  practice  with  the  finger  tips  of  the  left 
hand  increased  the  fineness  of  discrimination  of  the  finger  tips  of 
the  right  hand,  but  not  of  the  left  fore-arm.  Practice  with  the 
third  phalanx  increased  the  fineness  of  discrimination  on  the  first 
phalanx. 

Also  of  interest  are  the  investigations  of  Urbantschisch,  of 
Epstein  and  of  Vogt  cited  by  Coover  and  Angel14.  Urbantsch- 
isch found  that  a  sound  stimuls  increased  the  sensitivity  of  the 
subject  for  visual,  gustatory,  olfactory  and  tactile  stimuli.  Sim- 
ilar results  are  reported  by  Epstein  in  regard  to  the  relation  be- 
tween auditory  and  visual  stimuli.  Vogt  showed  that  habitua- 
tion  to  distractions  in  one  situation  could  be  carried  over  to  other 
fields. 

A  piece  of  work  bearing  less  directly  on  the  question  of  trans- 
fer but  yet  indicating  something  in  regard  to  mental  correlations 
which  may  be  variously  interpreted,  was  recently  conducted  at  the 
University  of  Illinois  by  H.  L.  Kietz  and  Imogene  Shade15.  This 
concerned  itself  with  inquiring  into  "the  facts  of  correlation  be- 
tween the  efficiency  of  students  in  mathematics  and  their  efficien- 
cy in  (1)  foreign  languages,  (2)  natural  science".  The  method  of 
investigation  may  be  characterized  in  a  general  way  as  that  of  Gal- 
ton  and  Pearson.  The  source  of  the  data  is  the  records  of  the  reg- 
istrar of  the  University  of  Illinois.  The  results  of  the  investiga- 
tion are,  in  brief,  that  "a  high  correlation  exists  between  efficiency 
in  mathematics  and  natural  sciences,  and  also  between  efficiency 

™Bericnt  d.  fc.  sachs..  O-es.  d.  WissenscHaft,  1858. 
U0p.  tit.  p.  328. 

"Correlation  of  efficiency  in  mathematics  and  efficiency  in  other  subjects. 
The  Univ.  of  Illinois  Studies,  Vol.  VI.,  No.  10,  (1908). 


THE  DOGMA  OF  FORMAL  DISCIPLINE  15 

in  mathematics  and  foreign  languages".  While  the  results  here 
do  not  in  any  way  indicate  whether  there  has  been  a  transfer  of 
training  in  mathematics  to  the  other  subjects  they  seem  to  show 
that  studies  as  far  apart  as  mathematics  and  foreign  language 
evidently  have  many  points  in  common,  so  that  training  in  one 
subject  might  very  well  be  made  effective  in  the  other. 

I  have  reserved  for  consideration  until  the  last,  one  of  the 
most  extensive  and  important  series  of  experiments  relating  to  the 
question  of  transfer  of  training16,  namely  those  of  Ebert  and 
Meumann.  These  tests,  though  with  a  greatly  improved 
technique  and  with  a  larger  number  of  subjects,  are  in  gen- 
eral like  the  pioneer  investigations  conducted  by  James, 
The  results,  however,  are  decidedly  different  and  the  conclusions 
arrived  at  by  Meumann  are  on  the  whole  at  variance  with  those  of 
James.  At  the  beginning  of  the  Meumann  tests,  the  memories  of 
the  subjects  were  determined  for  nonsense  syllables,  numbers, 
letters,  one-syllabled  substantives,  Italian  words,  poetic  words, 
prose  words,  and  visual  signs.  In  the  training  series  which  fol- 
lowed, the  subjects  were  given  nonsense  syllables  to  memorize  and 
then  the  original  test  series  was  repeated  to  determine  improve- 
ment, if  any,  both  for  immediate  recall  and  for  permanent  reten- 
tion. In  some  cases  the  experiment  was  carried  still  farther,  a 
second  training  series  followed  by  another  test  series  being  intro- 
duced. The  special  training  with  nonsense  syllables  evidently  in- 
creased the  ability  of  the  subjects  to  memorize  and  retain  the 
materials  of  the  test  series.  The  amount  of  the  transfer  was 
found  to  be  great.  The  ability  to  memorize  philosophic  prose,  for 
example,  showed  an  increase  of  70  per  cent  after  the  practice  se- 
ries, and  to  memorize  visual  signs  55  per  cent.  Ebert  and  Meu- 
mann consider  that  increased  power  of  attending,  increase  in  vol- 
untary effort,  improvement  in  the  technique  of  learning  and  gen- 
eral decrease  in  discomfort  and  tediousness  are  the  chief  auxiliary 
causes  for  the  improvement  after  practice.  They  believe,  how- 
ever, that  beyond  these  conditions  and  fundamental  to  the  process 
of  transfer,  lies  a  sympathetic  interaction  of  allied  memory  func- 
tions, based  on  an  assumed  psychophysical  activity.  The  exist- 
ence of  such  an  uncertain  relationship  is  denied  by  the  reviewers 
of  this  piece  of  work.  Both  Mtiller  and  Dearborn,  who  have  criti- 
cized these  experiments,  believe  that  the  transfer  can  be  best  ex- 
plained by  considering  the  so-called  auxiliary  aids  as  the  sole  cause 
of  the  results  obtained.  Be  this  as  it  may,  Ebert  and  Meumann, 

18 Arch.  f.  d.  gesamte  Psychol..  Vol.  IV. 


16  UNIVERSITY  OF  ILLINOIS 

experienced  investigators  of  high  scholarship,  have  found  that 
so  mechanical  a  procedure  as  memorizing  nonsense  syllables  has 
a  pronounced  effect  on  learning  in  general.  Whatever  the  explan- 
ation may  be,  the  results  seem  beyond  reasonable  dispute,  both  on 
account  of  the  ability  of  the  investigators  and  the  unequivocal 
nature  of  their  findings. 

I  have  spent,  perhaps,  an  undue  amount  of  time  in  attempt- 
ing to  present  the  above  experimental  evidence.  It  has  been  my 
wish,  however,  to  avoid  as  much  as  possible,  speculation,  hypothe- 
sis, and  conjecture,  and  get  down  to  the  basis  of  undoubted  facts. 
There  can  be  no  dispute,  I  think,  in  regard  to  the  significance  of 
these  facts.  Transfer  is  indicated  in  practically  all  of  the  experi- 
ments, and  in  some,  particularly  in  the  last  described,  this  transfer 
is  striking.  The  only  investigations  which  showed  no  transfer 
were  those  of  James,  performed  admittedly  under  unsatisfactory 
conditions,  and  the  tests  of  Bagley  in  the  Montana  Normal 
School.  Bagley's  experiments,  however,  when  continued  under 
somewhat  different  conditions,  as  conducted  by  Kuediger,  show 
clear  evidence  of  transfer.  Of  the  remaining  experiments  those 
of  Thorndike  show  the  least  positive  result. 

I  take  it  that  in  the  light  of  all  the  evidence  there  cannot  be 
the  slightest  doubt  that  practice  effects  may  be,  and  generally  are, 
transferred  from  one  set  of  activities  to  another.  The  extent  of 
such  transfer  and  the  conditions  under  which  it  takes  place  are, 
however,  matters  for  further  investigation  and  of  great  pedagogi- 
cal significance.  Whether  the  results  are  due  to  transfer  of  iden- 
tical elements  (Thorndike)  ;  to  improvement  of  habitual  methods 
of  recording  facts  (James) ;  to  training  the  attention  and  will- 
power (Scripture  and  Davis)  ;  to  devesting  the  essential  process 
of  the  unessential  factors,  greater  habituation  and  more  economic- 
al adaptation  of  attention  (Coover  and  Angell) ;  to  the  effective 
use  of  mental  imagery  and  properly  controlled  attention  (Frank- 
er);  to  the  development  of  ideals  (Bagley  and  Kuediger)  ; 
to  general  improvement  in  technique  of  learning,  attention  and 
will-power,  but  chiefly  to  a  sympathetic  interaction  of  allied  mem- 
ory functions  (Ebert  and  Meumann),  or  to  some  other  factors  as 
yet  not  analyzed  out,  may  still  be  a  matter  of  investigation  and 
debate.  My  own  personal  opinion  is  that  practically  all  of  these 
are  more  or  less  important  elements  in  the  transfer. 


THE  DOGMA  OF  FORMAL  DISCIPLINE  17 

III.     THE  POSSIBILITIES  OF  FORMING  A  GENERAL 

HABIT. 

Although  the  evidence  of  transfer  seems  unequivocal  the  ex- 
treme anti-formalist  may  still  insist  that  the  results  of  the  investi- 
gations above  cited  are  misleading  and  declare  on  theoretical 
grounds  that  any  general  transfer  of  training  is  impossible,  since 
there  is  no  such  thing  as  a  "generalized"  habit.  As  Bagley  puts 
it,  "A  simple  habit  is  a  specific  response  to  a  specific  stimulus ;  a 
generalized  habit  would  be  a  specific  response  common  to  a  num- 
ber of  different  stimuli'7.  As  such,  "the  term  is  a  psychological 
absurdity".  In  a  similar  vein  Thorndike  asserts  that  "the  mind  is 
on  its  dynamic  side  a  machine  for  making  particular  reactions  to 
particular  situations.  It  works  in  great  detail,  adapting  itself  to 
special  data  of  which  it  has  had  experience".  The  case  seems 
very  simple.  A  habit  is  a  definite  response  to  a  definite  stimulus, 
and  as  all  training  tends  to  the  formation  of  habits,  there  can  be 
no  training  that  is  not  specific.  This  line  of  reasoning  might  seem 
all  very  well  if  it  did  not  prove  too  much.  If  habit  is  of  this  de- 
cidedly specific  character,  then  it  would  seem  to  follow  of  neces- 
sity that  the  aim  of  the  process  of  learning  is  merely  to  make  facile 
and  subconscious  those  things  which  have  already  been  done  after 
a  fashion,  and  not  to  prepare  the  individual  properly  to  react  to 
essentially  new  situations.  If  this  were  the  case  we  should  be  in- 
a  sorry  plight  indeed.  We  should  be  obliged  to  revise  all  our 
former  notions  of  an  education,  and  substitute  for  our  present 
procedure  a  narrow  and  illiberal  training,  habituating  the  pupil 
to  a  limited  sphere  of  predetermined  activities.  The  child  would 
be  but  little  better  off,  then,  in  his  learning  processes  than  is  the 
brute,  who  manifestly  is  trained  in  just  such  a  manner  as  de- 
scribed and  is  in  no  sense  educated.  Thorndike  attempts  to  es- 
cape from  this  obvious  difficulty  by  admitting  the  transfer  of 
training  through  elements  identical  in  two  situations. 

Such  a  transfer  in  the  simplest  form  is  well  illustrated  by  a 
series  of  experiment  carried  on  several  years  ago  in  the  psycholog- 
ical laboratory  of  the  University  of  Illinois.  In  these  experi- 
ments three  dogs  among  other  animals  were  tested  in  regard  to 
their  ability  to  discriminate  between  various  colors.  Throughout 
most  of  the  experiments  a  standard  red  was  the  color  which  they 
were  trained  to  associate  with  the  obtaining  of  food.  This  color 
was  painted  on  the  food-box,  and  the  dogs  soon  formed  the  habit 
of  reacting  directly  to  this  red-box  stimulus,  thus  obtaining  their 


18  UNIVERSITY  OF  ILLINOIS 

food.  Later  one  of  the  dogs  was  tested  to  see  how  far  he  could  as- 
sociate the  color  red  when  presented  not  merely  on  a  box  of  a  cer- 
tain size  and  appearance  but  on  various  receptacles  and  under 
various  conditions,  with  the  obtaining  of  food.  Gradually  this 
dog  was  trained  to  recognize  the  color  red  under  varied  condi- 
tions as  the  food  signal.  Thus,  quite  mechanically,  he  seems  to 
have  transferred  his  habit  from  one  situation  to  another  through 
the  identity  of  the  color  element  in  the  various  situations.  Such 
a  transfer  is  not  of  a  high  type,  nor  is  it  very  promising  from  the 
standpoint  of  educational  procedure.  The  possibilities  of  getting 
much  general  training  through  the  identity  of  objective  elements 
in  a  total  situation  do  not  seem  to  be  great.  It  should  be  stated, 
however,  that  Thorndike  understands  by  identical  elements  not 
only  identity  of  the  objective  stimulus  as  in  the  cease  of  the  food- 
box  stimulus,  but  the  identity  of  those  elements  that  constitute 
the  reaction  to  a  stimulus,  the  identity  of  adjustment  in  two  sit- 
uations. 

Bagley  has  got  around  the  difficulty  much  better  by  emphasiz- 
ing the  identity  or  similarity  of  certain  subjective  elements  in  a 
situation  (indeed  he  would  define  a  situation  in  terms  of  con- 
scious meaning  rather  than  in  terms  of  objective  elements)  in 
his  doctrine  of  a  transfer  through  the  creation  of  ideals.  He  says 
in  regard  to  the  habit  of  neatness,  for  example,  that  "those  who 
appear  to  carry  this  habit  over  from  one  department  of  life  to 
another  really  carry  over  the  ideal  of  neatness."  The  importance 
of  this  general  principle,  thus  formulated  by  Bagley,  cannot  be. 
overestimated,  yet  I  am  inclined  to  believe  that  neither  this,  nor 
the  principle  of  identical  elements  as  set  forth  by  Thorndike  is 
sufficient  to  explain  all  there  is  in  the  transfer,  nor  to  exhaust  the 
possibilities  of  a  general  training. 

In  several  senses  I  believe  that  we  are  warranted  in  speaking 
of  a  generalized  habit.  Such  an  expression  seems  to  me  admis- 
sable  under  the  following  conditions : 

(1. )  When  the  specific  stimulus  that  calls  forth  a  specific  re- 
action is  common  to  a  large  variety  of  situations,  which  situations 
may  have  little  in  common  beyond  the  presence  in  each  of  the  spe- 
cific stimulus.  In  the  case  of  the  food-box  reaction,  for  example, 
described  above,  the  color  red  was  the  element  in  the  many  situa- 
tions which  produced  under  varying  circumstances  the  fool-seek- 
ing response.  Such  a  reaction  would  be  general  in  the  sense  that 
it  could  take  place  under  may  objective  conditions,  and  thus  as  far 
as  the  environment  is  concerned,  constitute  a  generalized  re- 


THE  DOGMA  OF  FORMAL  DISCIPLINE  19 

spouse  through  the  similarity  or  identity  of  a  single  element  in 
that  environment.  Take  another  example  of  a  similar  sort :  The 
soldier  who  has  learned  to  come  to  attention  at  the  word  of  com- 
mand will  do  so  on  the  parade  ground,  the  battlefield  or  the  street. 
It,  of  course,  may  be  said  that  for  the  soldier  who  so  responds  the 
actual  situation  is  the  same  under  these  varying  conditions. 
However,  this  is  true  only  as  far  as  the  dominant  element  in  the 
situation  remains  similar,  this  dominant  element  being  deter- 
mined not  only  by  its  objective  importance  to  the  ordinary  ob- 
server, but  by  the  fact  that  the  soldier  does  thus  react  under  va- 
rious circumstances  (the  reaction  thus  being  the  criterion  of  iden- 
tity of  stimulus. )  For  the  ordinary  observer,  however,  this  dom- 
inant element  may  be  of  but  slight  importance,  and  thus  in  his  re- 
lations with  his  fellowmen,  the  soldier  may  appear  to  have  ac- 
quired a  generalized  habit  and  for  all  practical  purposes  his  reac- 
tion may  be  treated  as  such. 

(2)  There  is  another  class  of  habitual  reactions  which  do 
not  seem  to  be  called  forth  by  any  definite  objective  stimulus,  but 
which  appear  under  a  large  variety  of  objective  conditions  in 
which  no  single  common  element  can  be  found.  These  latter  cases 
arise  when  the  reaction  is  under  the  dominance  of  a  mood  or  emo- 
tion that  so  colors  the  objective  environment  that  several  different 
stimuli  may  call  forth  the  habitual  response.  A  person,  for  ex- 
ample, of  a  choleric  disposition  may  have  established  a  very  defi- 
nite set  of  reactions  which  habitually  expresses  his  angry  moods. 
The  insignificant  external  causes  which  set  off  this  definite  re- 
sponse may  vary  greatly  and  it  may  thus  be  urged,  that  not  one 
stimulus,  but  many,  are  capable  of  producing  the  reaction.  In 
cases  of  morbid  pity,  irrational  fear,  religious  enthusiasm,  and 
the  like,  the  object  which  gives  expression  to  these  moods  is  ap- 
parently indifferent.  Of  course  it  may  be  replied  that  the  mood  it- 
self is  due  to  a  complex  of  bodily  stimuli  (such  as  those  set  forth 
in  the  James-Lange  theory  of  the  emotions)  and  what  really  is 
true  is  that  in  all  the  habitual  expressions  of  the  emotion  there 
exist  certain  common  and  definite  stimuli  discharging  themselves 
into  the  higher  centres,  and  that  this  second  class  of  habitual  re- 
sponses really  belongs  with  the  first  class,  mainly  of  response  to 
identical  stimuli.  Whether  this  is  theoretically  true  or  not,  we 
again  have,  and  to  a  greater  extent  than  in  the  cases  cited  under 
the  first  class,  what  may  be  practically  considered  as  a  very  gen- 
eral response  to  a  large  number  of  different  environmental  condi- 
tions. 


20  UNIVERSITY  OF  ILLINOIS 

(3)  We  further  have  in  any  definite  reaction  to  a  given  sit- 
uation not  merely  one  elemental  adjustment,  but  generally  many 
both  positive  and  negative.  This  the  following  discussion  will  at- 
tempt to  make  clear : 

Let  us  suppose  that  a  child  in  the  school  room  is  being  taught 
to  correctly  form  the  letter  a  in  his  copy  book.  Here  we  have  the 
example  of  training  in  a  special  habit.  We  have  a  certain  defi- 
nite stimulus  of  sight,  namely  the  letter  a  of  the  copy  book,  which 
constitutes  the  essential  stimulus  to  which  there  is  a  specific  re- 
sponse, the  writing  of  the  letter  a.  This  gradually  becomes  more 
and  more  an  habitual  process  and  we  have  set  up  a  definite  habit 
of  stimulus  and  response.  I  wish  to  submit,  however,  that  besides 
this  specific  stimulus  of  the  written  or  printed  a  on  the  copy  book, 
there  are  other  stimuli  which  constitute  the  total  situation  which 
might  function  just  as  well  for  copying  b,  or  any  other  letter,  as  a,. 
We  have  not  merely  the  specific  a-copying  reaction  but  we  have 
as  well  a  more  general  reaction  based  on  the  seeing  of  the  copy 
book,  the  "feel"  of  the  pen  in  the  hand,  etc.,  which  may  be  termed 
the  "copy-book"  reaction ;  beyond  this  we  have  a  still  more  general 
group  of  motor  expressions  and  exhibitions  which  constitute  the 
"school"  reaction  as  such  and  differentiate  it  from  the  "home"  re- 
action, for  example.  Now  in  the  copying  of  the  letter  a  all  these 
various  reactions  are  involved,  but  only  a  very  small  part  of  the 
total  reaction  functions  solely  for  the  a-copying  habit ;  much  of  it 
might  function  equally  well  for  the  reading-habit,  or  the  number- 
work  habit.  There  is  then  a  considerable  part  of  the  a-copying 
habit  that  is  not  specific  in  the  sense  that  it  is  confined  to  the  one 
particular  reaction  of  copying  the  a.  It  is  general  in  the  sense 
that  it  concerns  itself  with  many  other  school  activities.  For  ex- 
ample, the  ignoring  of  the  noises  on  the  street,  the  holding  of  the 
body  in  the  proper  position  at  the  desk,  etc.,  are  reactions  that 
apply  to  various  school  situations  other  than  the  one  concerned 
with  the  business  of  copying  the  a. 

Perhaps  this  thought  may  be  made  more  clear  by  the  accom- 
panying diagram.  In  this  the  script  letters  a,  b,  y,  z,  represent  cer- 
tain specific  activities  in  which  the  pupil  is  being  trained,  a,  how- 
ever, involves  the  more  general  adjustment  W,  which  in  turn  in- 
volves the  still  more  general  adjustment  S.  Now  while  in  the 
activity  a,  W  and  S  are  both  involved,  these  may  also  be  involved 
equally  well  in  still  other  activities.  For  example,  W  is  involved 
among  other  things  in  the  activity  b,  while  S  is  involved  not  only 
in  W,  but  in  R  as  well.  To  be  more  concrete,  let  us  suppose  that 


THE  DOGMA  OF  FORMAL  DISCIPLINE  21 


22  UNIVERSITY  OF  ILLINOIS 

a  represents  the  a-copying  habit,  and  W  the  writing  habit  and 
S  the  school  habit,  while  E  represents  the  reading  habit  and  y  the 
habit  of  reading  in  verse.  Now  it  is  quite  obvious  that  in  learn- 
ing to  copy  a,  the  other  higher  habits,  if  they  have  not  already 
been  formed,  will  of  necessity  gradually  be  set  up.  Suppose  the 
child  should  begin  his  activities  in  school  (which  of  course,  he 
actually  would  not)  by  learning  to  copy  the  letter  a.  In  this  ac- 
tivity he  would  gradually  acquire  the  more  fundamental  habit  of 
paying  attention  in  the  school  room  and  of  the  general  technique 
of  learning  to  write.  Thus  in  learning  to  copy  the  a,  he  would 
have  also  acquired  a  stock  of  habits  which  could  be  transferred  to 
other  school  situations.  In  other  words,  learning  to  copy  a 
would  constitute  a  general  training  as  well  as  a  specific  habit- 
forming  activity. 

So  we  are  justified  in  speaking  of  general  habits  of  attending, 
or  of  thinking,  or  of  willing,  although  as  Bagley  would  urge  with 
justice,  attention,  thought  and  resolution  are  mental  states,  which 
as  highly  conscious,  raise  the  individual  above  the  plane  of  habit- 
ual activities,  and  make  it  possible  for  him  to  respond  in  a  new 
way  to  a  new  situation.  The  fact,  however,  that  he  does  attend, 
has  the  attitude  of  attention,  is  due  to  certain  specific  tendencies 
toward  reaction  which  have  been  gradually  acquired  and  made 
reflex.  The  attending  to  the  dictation  of  the  teacher,  for  ex- 
ample, brings  into  the  consciousness  of  the  child  a  situation  to 
which  he  may  intelligently  react,  but  the  possibility  of  getting  the 
attitude  of  this  attention  is  largely  dependent  on  many  subcon- 
scious and  habitual  factors,  such  as  ignoring  unessential  stimuli, 
disregarding  tedium,  and  in  general  having  developed  a  technique 
of  learning.  These  elements  have  been  emphasized  by  most  of  the 
investigators  above  cited  as  essential  in  the  process  of  transfer. 

If  we  consider  again  the  diagram  just  discussed  we  shall  be 
able  to  see  how  it  explains  some  of  the  facts  of  transfer  or  its  lack, 
as  shown  in  investigations  above  considered. 

In  one  of  the  tests  of  Thorndike,  for  example,  the  subjects 
were  given  practice  in  marking  the  words  on  a  printed  page  con- 
taining the  two  letters  e  and  s.  Before  and  after  this  training 
these  subjects  were  tested  in  marking  the  words  containing  other 
combinations  of  letters,  misspelled  words,  different  parts  of 
speech,  etc.  Improvement  in  the  second  test  series  was  measured 
by  increased  speed  and  accuracy.  In  general  speed  was  more 
likely  to  be  improved  than  accuracy.  The  reason  for  this  I  think 


THE  DOGMA  OF  FORMAL  DISCIPLINE  23 

is  perfectly  obvious  in  terms  of  our  diagram,  as  can  be  seen  from 
the  following.  Suppose  that  a  represents  the  practice  in  marking 
the  words  containing  e  and  8,  while  5  represents  the  marking  of 
words  containing  other  letters,  etc.  It  is  quite  obvious  that  the 
specific  marking  habit  acquired  in  the  first  series,  enters  into  con- 
flict in  a  certain  sense  with  the  marking  of  words  containing  m 
and  I,  for  example,  since  the  attention  is  turned  from  words  of  one 
general  make-up  in  the  practice  series  to  those  of  another  com- 
position, in  the  test  series.  Thus  the  acquired  tendency  to  mark 
words  containing  e  and  s  will  actually  tend  to  inhibit  the  marking 
of  words  containing  m  and  I.  Hence  there  may  be  a  falling  off  in 
accuracy.  On  the  other  hand  the  more  general  reaction  of  mark- 
ing words  has  been  trained  at  the  same  time  as  the  habit  of  mark- 
ing specific  words.  This  latter  phase  of  the  habit  (which  Thorn- 
dike  would  term  an  identical  element,  but  which  I  prefer  to  call 
a  more  general  attitude)  may  be  transferred  from  the  marking  of 
one  kind  of  word  to  the  other.  This  might  result  in  greater  speed 
and  at  the  same  time  diminished  accuracy.  I  venture  to  say  that 
if  a  subject  had  been  trained  to  high  efficiency  in  the  word-marking 
habit,  further  training  in  marking  certain  specific  words  would 
tend  to  exercise  a  negative  influence  in  marking  other  words 
similar  to  them.  The  nearer  the  activities  were  alike  in  this  case, 
the  greater  would  be  the  distraction  of  attention,  and  the  greater 
the  falling  off  in  efficiency. 

It  would  seem  probable  for  this  reason  that  highly  trained 
laboratory  subjects  would  show  less  general  effect  of  training 
than  would  naive  subjects,  and  that  adults  would  show  less  ef- 
fect than  would  children.  The  laboratory  subject  who  has  mas- 
tered the  technique  of  giving  himself  over  to  the  test  at  hand,  who 
knows  how  to  hold  his  attention  down  to  the  minimum  of  fluctua- 
tion and  to  overcome  the  loss  of  interest  arising  through  ennui, 
can  expect  to  get  little  general  training  in  carrying  on  some  spe- 
cific activity  like  judging  areas,  tapping  at  dots  on  paper,  or 
learning  nonsense  syllables.  The  greatest  possibilities  of  trans- 
ferring general  attitudes  of  attention,  thinking,  etc.,  except  as 
conscious  ideals  or  purposes,  lie,  then,  with  untrained  adults  and 
with  children,  especially  with  the  latter.  This  makes  the  edu- 
cative process  so  hopeful  where  children  are  concerned  and  rela- 
tively hopeless  with  adults.  It  is  not  that  adults  cannot  learn 
new  habits,  as  James  maintains  in  his  famous  chapter  on  habit; 
it  is  that  they  cannot  generalize  these  habits,  that  makes  the  man 
of  forty  an  old  fogy. 


24  UNIVERSITY  OF  ILLINOIS 

Transfer  of  training  is  then  possible  in  the  ways  indicated: 
(1)  Where  a  single  element  to  which  a  specific  response  is  made 
functions  under  various  environmental  conditions  because  it 
is  a  common  element  in  these  various,  and  otherwise  to  a  greater 
or  less  degree,  dissimilar  environments;  (2)  When  a  dominant 
mood  or  emotion  so  colors  various  environments  that  a  charac- 
teristic response  is  obtained  without  identity  of  any  one  objec- 
tive condition;  (3)  Where  a  single  response  in  reality  involves 
other  and  more  general  adjustments.  (4)  It  is  also  possible, 
as  Bagley  suggests,  through  making  the  end  of  the  activity  a 
clearly  conscious  ideal.  In  this  case  the  transfer  takes  place  by 
a  direct  carrying  over  by  consciousness  not  of  the  activity  itself, 
but  of  the  purpose  of  the  activity,  to  another  field.  This  transfer 
may  be  represented  in  the  diagram  by  the  dotted  lines  from  a  to  5 
from  a  to  z,  and  so  on,  showing  a  direct  transfer  without  involv- 
ing the  adjustments  at  W,  S,  or  R.  To  illustrate  by  a  concrete 
example,  the  habit  of  attention  in  school  (S)  may  function  quite 
unconsciously,  from  having  been  acquired  in  the  reading  of  poetry 
(y) ;  in  connection  with  the  writing  of  certain  letters  in  the  copy- 
book (b),  or  it  may  function  rather  because  the  school-attention 
attitude  has  been  made  a  conscious  ideal  in  connection  with  y 
and  is  therefore  carried  over  as  an  end  of  action  rather  than  as  a 
habit.  This  general  scheme  with  this  modification  suggests,  I 
believe,  all  the  possibilities  of  transfer;  general  moods  and  atti- 
tudes that  have  grown  up  quite  unconsciously  as  well  as  devel- 
oped conscious  purposes  finding  their  place  in  the  transfer. 
Whether  beyond  this  there  is  some  mysterious  sort  of  harmony, 
or  sympathetic  vibration,  in  the  nervous  system  that  makes  it 
possible  for  one  habit  to  set  up  another  without  a  transfer  in 
the  ways  suggested,  we  do  not  know.  But  if  it  exists  it  is  buried 
at  present  so  far  below  the  threshold  of  consciousness  that  it  has 
no  practical  educational  significance  and  need  not  be  considered 
here. 

IV.     EULES  FOE  SECUEING  TEANSFEE. 

The  possibility  of  a  general  training  is  thus  seemingly  es- 
tablished both  in  theory  and  in  fact,  and  it  becomes  the  business 
of  education  to  consider  how  such  a  training  can  best  be  secured. 
I  believe  that  it  is  possible  in  the  light  of  all  the  evidence  present- 
ed on  the  subject  of  transfer  to  lay  down  with  tolerable  certainty 
a  few  rules  of  procedure. 

(1).     The  first  rule  should  be:     Make  those  specific  activi- 


THE  DOGMA  OF  FORMAL  DISCIPLINE  25 

ties  which  you  wish  to  transfer  the  object  of  thought.  Let  the 
significance  of  the  habit  and  its  general  bearings  become  known 
to  the  person  who  is  the  subject  of  the  training.  Bagley  has  em- 
phasized this  factor  in  training  in  his  doctrine  of  transference 
through  ideals,  and  the  experiments  of  Ruediger  seem  to  justify 
the  contention.  The  results  of  Judd,  who,  as  previously  stated, 
has  shown  that  practice  with  knowledge  has  a  value  in  the  trans- 
fer of  training  which  practice  without  knowledge  does  not  pos- 
sess, also  point  to  the  same  general  conclusion.  Likewise, 
Meumann  states  that  it  is  desirable  in  training  children  formally 
to  bring  to  their  attention  the  significance  of  such  training.  It 
further  should  be  said  that  our  knowledge  of  the  functioning  of 
the  nervous  system  is  in  strict  accord  with  this  general  position  in 
regard  to  transfer,  since  the  association  fibres  of  the  cortex  are  the 
ones  which  connect  various  sensory  and  motor  areas  and  their 
function  is  probably  primarily  related  to  the  higher  conscious 
processes. 

It  would  seem  then  that  we  have  a  definite  means  which  edu- 
cation can  pursue  in  formal  training,  and  this  means  removes  the 
criticism  that  such  training  is  merely  mechanical  and  deadening. 

(2)  Train  the  child  in  the  technique  of  learning  and  in  the 
processes  that  make  learning  effective  and  economical.  Nearly 
all  the  investigations  emphasize  the  value  of  properly  adapted  at- 
tention, of  satisfactory  physical  and  mental  attitudes  in  securing 
transfer.  Sustained  attention  should  be  developed  in  the  school 
training,  not  merely  for  the  sake  of  the  object  attended  to  (per- 
haps not  primarily  for  the  object's  sake),  but  rather  for  the  sake 
of  attention  itself.  The  whole  art  of  learning 


and  skillfully  controlled.  The  importance  of  right  method  of 
learning  has  been  emphasized  in  recent  years  largely  through  the 
work  of  Meumann.  It  appears  that  one  of  the  chiei^im^jofjedjl: 
-cation  should  bp  ffQ  fparh  the  cl^ld  how  to  a^flWP  frnnwli  Yhi  will) 
the  least  expenditure  of  time  and^je^,ej^y,^«^»{^Uble^^44^^JtS' 
retention  for  effective  use.  Personally  I  am  convinced  that  one 
of  the  greatest  needs  of  formal  training  in  this  connection  is  the 
development  of  the  child's  mental  imagery.  See  to  it  that  chil- 
dren can  employ  various  kinds  of  imagery  effectively,  develop 
the  imagery  for  form  and  for  color,  the  imagery  for  sounds  and 
for  kin  aesthetic  sensation  of  throat,  hand,  and  fingers  when  pos- 
sible. Many  a  poor  reader  cannot  visualize,  many  a  child  defici- 
ent in  nicety  of  motor  control  lacks  kinaesthetic  imagery;  all. 


26  UNIVERSITY  OF  ILLINOIS 

training  in  musical  notation  is  worthless  knowledge  unless  the 
child  has  a  fair  auditory  imagery. 

(3)  In  seeking  to  secure  transfer,  especially  where  pur- 
pose does  not  play  an  important  part,  see  to  it  that  the  stimulus 
which  is  to  call  forth  the  desired  reaction  is  such  that  it  may  be 
a  common  element  in  many  objective  situations.     If,  for  exam- 
ple, it  is  desired  to  promote  in  general  the  habit  of  observation,  it 
will  be  unwise  to  cultivate  this  habit  in  a  very  narrow  and  un- 
usual field  of  experience.     Habits  of  observation  may  doubtless 
be  secured  by  training  the  observer  to  give  careful  attention  to 
objects  appearing  under  the  miscroscope.     This  training  in  ob- 
servation will  on  the  whole  probably  have  less  possibilities  of 
transfer  to  other  fields  than  observation  cultivated  in  the  study 
of  more  common  objects  of  life,  such  as  those  of  plants  and  ani- 
mals that  are  often  met  with  in  the  daily  environment. 

(4)  Education  should  cultivate   through   specific   training 
general  emotional  attitudes.     Moods  and  feelings  often  are  the 
dominant  elements  in  a     situation  and     these    can    readily    be 
transferred,  I  believe.     The  child  who  has  the  proper  emotional 
attitude  toward  his  school  life  will  be  the  one  who  will  act  most 
capably  in  the  school  environment.     Education  should  see  to  it 
that  such  general  feeling  attitudes  as  docility,  respect  for  author- 
ity, eagerness  to  be  of  service,  and  the  like  are  developed  through 
the  school  training.  Such  attitudes,  unlike  the  ideals  which  Bag- 
ley  emphasizes,  need  not  (perhaps  should  not)  be  raised  to  full 
consciousness.     This,  I  believe,  has  a  bearing  on  moral  instruc- 
tion in  the  schools.     I  have  sometimes  thought  that  intellectuliz- 
ing  what  ought  to  be  an  emotional  attitude  is  a  dangerous  pro- 
cedure.    This  thought  has  significance  in  relation  to  temperance 
instruction  and  the  like.    I  think  that  the  question  may  well  be 
raised, — Has  not  the  instruction  on  the  effect  of  alcohol  and  nar- 
cotics the  tendency  to  make  the  child's  attitude  merely  a  matter 
of  intellect,  when  it  should  primarily  remain  one  of  feeling  and 
of  will? 

V.     THE  SUPERIOR     DISCIPLINARY     VALUE     OF 
PURE  AS  COMPARED  WITH  APPLIED  SCIENCE. 

Finally,  if  it  is  true  that  a  formal  training  is  possible,  and 
if  it  is  desirable  that  the  schools  furnish  such  a  training,  we  come 
back  to  the  old  question  as  to  whether  there  are  certain  studies 
that  are  better  suited  than  others  to  offer  this  discipline.  It 


THE  DOGMA  OF  FORMAL  DISCIPLINE  27 

would  seem  probable  that  there  are  subjects  which  either  because 
of  the  nature  of  their  subject  matter,  or  because  of  the  better  tech- 
nique that  goes  with  their  instruction  are  today  more  valuable 
than  others  from  the  standpoint  of  mental  training.  Other 
studies  which  are  not  now  so  well  developed  will,  perhaps,  some 
day  take  the  place  of  mathematics,  or  natural  science,  or  foreign 
language,  but  today  they  are  less  valuable  from  a  disciplinary 
standpoint. 

Just  at  present  the  most  important  controversy  concerning 
educational  values  is  being  carried  on  between  those  who  advocate 
the  advantages  of  applied  science  and  those  who  hold  to  the  great- 
er value  of  pure  science.  The  proper  solution  of  the  relative  im- 
portance of  the  two  spheres  of  human  knowledge  will  doubtless 
have  much  to  do  with  the  development  of  the  most  valuable  cur- 
riculum of  studies  in  our  secondary  schools  and  colleges.  I  am 
inclined  to  believe  that  at  least  as  far  as  mental  discipline  is 
concerned,  pure  science  has  much  in  its  favor  as  a  subject  of 
instruction.  In  a  forthcoming  volume,  Bagley  emphasizes 
the  probable  superiority  of  pure  mathematics  over  applied  mat- 
hematics with  its  utilitarian  ends  which  tend  to  color  every  other 
consideration ;  thus  obscuring  the  ideals  of  accuracy  and  rigidity 
that  the  pure  science  teaches.  "Applied  mathematics",  says 
Bagley,  "will  inevitably  demand  a  quantity  rather  than  qual- 
ity   With  the  better  and  more  intelligent  students,  the 

discipline  may  come  in  spite  of  haste.  With  the  average  student, 
the  longer  and  more  penetrating  processes  from  which  the  percep- 
tion of  the  unique  values  of  mathematical  reasoning  will  emerge, 
will  be  omitted". 

What  is  true  in  regard  to  pure  mathematics  as  compared 
with  applied  mathematics  is,  I  believe,  true  in  regard  to  other 
pure  sciences  as  compared  with  other  applied  sciences.  The  ap- 
plication of  a  science  tends  to  emphasize  certain  human  values 
more  or  less  remote  from  the  value  of  the  science  as  science; 
hence,  to  restrict  the  field  of  the  inquiry  to  those  phases  of  the 
science  that  seem  to  relate  most  definitely  to  economic  and  other 
more-or-less  narrow  human  values.  With  this  point  of  view  the 
investigator  is  apt  not  only  to  lose  the  ideals  of  rigidity  which 
Bagley  points  out,  but  also  the  ideal  of  truth  merely  as  truth; 
truth  that  is  self-sufficient  and  confident,  that  knows  that  what- 
ever is  true  is  human  and  whatever  is  untrue  or  partially  true  can 
never  have  ultimate  worth  in  human  experience.  This  ideal 


28  UNIVERSITY  OF  ILLINOIS 

gained  in  one  pure  science  and  made  a  conscious  end  of  endeavor 
in  all  fields  of  human  experience  has  a  rich  promise  for  the  fu- 
ture. The  great  discoveries  of  science  have  been  made  by  those 
who  were  primarily  scientists,  who  had  the  scientific  ideal.  Ap- 
plied science  comes  later  and  uses  the  knowledge  of  a  Helmholtz 
playing  with  the  opthalmoscope  or  a  Darwin,  in  the  spirit  of 
scientific  curiosity,  collecting  specimens  and  data  from  which  has 
developed  the  theory  of  evolution  with  its  countless  applications 
to  human  life.  The  mere  facts  of  science  are  worth  much;  rela- 
tively less,  however,  in  the  first  years  of  study  before  specializa- 
tion has  begun  in  the  technical  school  or  university;  but  the  fact 
as  fact  has  slight  disciplinary  value  unless  from  it  grows  the 
spirit  of  curiosity,  the  emotional  ideal,  which  the  Greeks  realized 
gave  birth  to  all  knowledge,  and  through  which  modern  education 
has  achieved  such  results.  My  own  conclusions  would  be  that  pure 
science  is  of  greater  disciplinary  value  because  (1)  through  the 
facts  which  it  presents,  ideal  of  procedure  and  of  truth  may  be  de- 
veloped which  function  in  a  wider  human  experience,  greatly  to 
the  uplift  of  the  race;  (2)  the  content  and  method  of  pure  science 
is  such  that  it  has  a  broader  field  of  application  than  has  applied 
science,  and  can  function  as  an  identical  or  similar  element  in 
more  situations  than  can  applied  science;  (3)  the  emotion 
which  the  pure  seeking  after  truth  arouse  is  higher  and 
less  likely  to  be  deadened  by  other  emotions  than  are  the  ideals 
of  economic  improvement  and  social  betterment,  which  are  the 
aims  of  an  applied  science.  These  latter  are  apt  to  conflict  with 
each  other  and  to  obscure  the  greater  issue.  Truth  has  but  one 
aim,  to  know  itself;  it  has  a  greater  emotional  uplift  and  is  one 
of  the  fundamental  passions  of  the  human  race;  as  fundamental 
as  the  economic  and  social  needs,  and  capable  of  ranges  of  flight 
toward  the  ideal  that  are  denied  the  other  instinctive  longings. 

We  must  realize  that  not  every  subject  that  has  utilitarian 
value,  or  that  excites  popular  interest,  is  for  that  reason  solely, 
a  fit  subject  for  instruction  in  the  schools.  Ultra-conservatism 
has  too  often  insisted  in  keeping  in  the  curriculum  those  studies 
that  have  long  since  ceased  to  be  vital;  on  the  other  hand  we  are 
equally  in  danger,  particularly  at  the  present  moment,  of  going  to 
the  other  and  equally  fatal  extreme.  There  are  so  many  special 
interests  that  just  now  seem  to  be  clamoring  for  recognition,  prac- 
tical, humanitarian,  aesthetic,  that  our  school  programmes  are 
in  danger  of  being  over-crowded  with  a  variety  of  subjects  which 


THE  DOGMA  OF  FORMAL  DISCIPLINE  29 

cannot  well  take  the  place  in  point  of  mental  training  of  those 
which  have  for  years  been  firmly  established  in  the  curri- 
culum. The  very  multiplicity  of  the  subjects  that  have  enriched 
our  programmes  offers  a  distraction  and  furnishes  a  training  in 
dispersed  rather  than  concentrated  attention,  a  training  which  is 
not  needed  and  should  not  be  desired.  The  trend  of  popular  opin- 
ion is  such  that  the  new  must  come  in,  and  I  am  far  from  main- 
taining that  this  opinion  is  not  on  the  whole  sound ;  but  let  us  see 
to  it  that  this  new  element  is  assigned  its  proper  place  and  given 
its  just  value.  In  this  time  of  rapid  change  we  need  sanity  in 
educational  doctrine  and. practice  as  scarcely  ever  before. 


UNIVERSITY  OF  CALIFORNIA  LIBRARY 
This  book  is  DUE  on  the  last  date  stamped  below, 
schedule:  25  cents  on  first  day  overdue    , 
50  cents  on  fourth  day  overdue 
One  dollar  on  seventh  day  overdue. 


17  Dec's? 


DEC!  6 195: 


INTEHLIBRARYIOAN 


':OC 

,UWV.O 


1  4  1991 


:' 


y   191947 

PEG  29  ^4l 

-.- 
f'l'ii   '    '•' 

19Jan5lCD 


JAN 


REC'D  LD 


BERK. 


YC  64005 


UNIVERSITY  OF  CALIFORNIA  LIBRARY 


.