Skip to main content

Full text of "Syllabus of lectures in theoretical biology"

See other formats


311 
B  62 


syllabus  of  Xectures 


IN 


ilbeoretical  Biology 


A- 


SYLLABUS   OF   LECTURES 


IN 


THEORETICAL  BIOLOGY 


BY 


ROBERT  PAYNE  BIGELOW,  Ph.  D. 


PREPARED    FOR   THE    USE    OF    STUDENTS    IN   THE  MASSACHU- 
SETTS  INSTUrUTE   OF   TECHNOLOGY. 


•  BOSTON: 
MASSACHUSETTS  INSTITUTE  OF  TECHNOLOGY 

I  896 


Copyright,  1896, 
By  Robert  P.  Biuki.ow. 


CONTENTS. 


I.    Introduction. 

1 .  Scope  of  biology. 

2.  Origin  of  the  science  of  biology. 

II.    Characteristics  of  living  matter. 

III.  Constitution  of  the  individual.     Organs  and  tissues,  Cell  theory. 

IV.  What  is  meant  by  "  Individual." 

1.  Kinds  of  individuality. 

2.  Degrees  of  individuality. 

3.  Limits  to  the  divisibility  of  the  individual. 

V.    Relations  between  the  parts  of  the  individual.    Analogy,  Homol- 
ogy, Symmetry. 

VI.    Origin  of  the  individual  (Morphogenesis). 

1 .  Abiogenesis  7>s.  biogenesis. 

2.  Evolution  Ts.  epigenesis. 

3.  The  advance  of  embryology. 

4.  Modern  theories  of  fertilization. 

5.  Phases  of  ontogeny. 

6.  Reaction  of  the  individual  to  its  environment. 

7.  Heredity  and  variation. 

i.  General  considerations, 

ii.  Phenomena  of  individual  variation, 

iii.  Phenomena  of  heredity, 

iv.  Theories  of  heredity  and  development. 

VII.    What  is  meant  i:)y  "  Species." 


37345 


VIII.    Origin  of  species  (I'hylogenesis). 

1.  Special  creation  7's.  organic  evolution. 

i.    Special  creation. 

ii.    Unity  of  Nature  as  shown  by  geology. 

iii.    Organic  evolution. 

a.    Meanings  of  the  word  "  Evolution.'' 
d.    Evidences  of  organic  evolution. 

2.  Theories  of  organic  evolution. 

i.    The  rise  of  the  theory  of  descent, 
ii.    The  theory  of  direct  modification, 
iii.    The  theory  of  selection, 
iv.    Neo-Lamarckians  ts.  Neo-Darwinians. 

3.  Examination  of  supposed  factors  in  organic  evolution. 

i.    \'ariation  and  heredity, 
ii.    Struggle  for  existence, 
iii.    Natural  selection. 

iv.    Panmixia  and  the  reversal  of  selection. 
V.    Sexual  selection. 
vi.    Isolation,  or  segregation, 
vii.    Inheritance  of  acquired  characters, 
viii.    Constitutional  tendency. 

4.  General  conclusions. 

IX.    Deductions  from  the  theory  of  evolution. 


I.    INTRODUCTION. 

1.  The  scope  of  Biology. 

2.  The  origin  of  the  Science  of  Biology. 

(See  Huxley.     Lecture  on  the  Study  of  Biology,  American  Ad- 
dresses, or  Collected  Essays,  Vol.  3,  pp.  262-293.) 

Natural     history    and   civil    history,    Bacon    (1561- 
1626),  and  HoBBES  (i  588-1679). 

Natural     philosophy    and    natural     history,     Buffon 
(i 707-1 778)  and  LiNN^us  (i 707-1 788). 

Physical  sciences  and  physiological  sciences,  Bichat, 
(Anatomic  Generale,  1801). 

Biologic,  Lamarck  (i8oi). 

Biologic,  Treviranus  (1802). 

3.  Our  object,  a  philosophical  review  of  Biology  as  a  whole. 


II.    CHARACTERISTICS    OF    LIVING    MATTER. 

(See  Hatschek,  Zoologie  pp.  i-ii;  Sedgwick  and  Wilson,  General 
Biology,  pp.  1-6;  Foster,  Physiology,  Introduction  last  edition;  Verworn, 
Allegemeine  Physiologic,  1895,  Trans,  by  Lee;  Howell,  American  Text- 
book of  Physiology,  Introduction,  1896.) 

Chemical  composition. 

Assimilation,  growth  and  reproduction. 

Contractility  and  irritability. 

The  organic  individual. 

Individuals  grouped  into  species. 


III.    THE    CONSTITUTION    OF   THE  INDIVIDUAL. 

(See  Tyson,  The  Cell  Doctrine,  Phila.  1S7S  ;  HALLiiiURTON,  Chemical 
Physiology,  pp.  183-216;  Huxley,  Review  of  the  Cell  Theory,  British  and 
Foreign  Medico-Chirurgical  Review.  Oct.  1853,  Vol.  12,  pp.  285-314;  Mar- 
SHALL,  Biological  Lectures,  pp.  159-191  ;  Hertwig,  The  Cell,  1895;  Wilson, 
The  Cell,  1896.) 

Partes  .similare.s  and  partes  dissimilares,  Aristotle 
{384-321,  B.  c). 

Organs  and   tissues,    Galen  (130-200);    Fallopius 
(1523-1562). 

Cellular  structure  of  plants,  Hooke  (1667);  Mal- 
piGHi    (1670);  and  Grew  (1672). 

The  fibre-theory,  Haller  (1757). 

Wolff's  theory  (1759). 

The  globular  theory  (1779-1842). 

Discovery  of  the  nucleus,  Robert  Brown  (1833). 

The  cell-theory,  Schleiden  and  Schwann  (1838). 

Sarkode,  Dujardin  (1835). 

Protoplasm,  Purkinje  (1840);  Hugo  von  Mohl 
(1846). 

The  protoplasm-theory.  Max  Schultze  (1861). 

Cell-division,  VON  MoiiL  (1835);  Nageli,  Kolliker 
(1844)  ;  Remak,  Virchow  (i860). 

Nuclear  division.  A,  Schneider  (1873);  Butschli 
(1875);  FoL   (1875);  Flemming  (1882). 

Attraction  sphere  and  centrosome.  Van  Beneden 
(1887);   BOVARI  (1888). 

The  inadequacy  of  the  cell-theory,  Huxley  (1853)  ; 
Whitman  (1893). 

The  search  for  organisms  more  elementary  than  the 
cell,  Brucke  (1861);  Altmann  (1890),  "Bio- 
blasts." 


IV.    WHAT    IS   MEANT    BY    "INDIVIDUAL." 

(See  Haeckel,  Ueber  die  Individualitiit  des  Thierkorpers,  Jenaische  Zeit- 
schrift,  Vol.  12,  1878;  Lillie,  Smallest  Parts  of  Stentor  Capable  of  Regenera- 
tion, Jour.  Morph.  Vol.  12,  1896,  pp.  239-249). 

1.  Kinds  of  individuality. 

i.   Physiological,  the  bion. 

ii.   Morphological,  the  viorphoti. 

2.  Degrees  of  individuality  in  morphons. 

i.   Plastid. 

a.   Cytode. 
/;.   Cell. 

ii.   Idorgan. 

a.  Organ. 

b.  Paramere. 

c.  Antimere. 

d.  Metamere. 

iii.   Person,  or  Zoon. 
iv.   Stock,  or  Cormus. 

3.  Limits    to    the   divisibility  of  the  individual,  Loeb,  Mor- 

gan, Lillie. 

V.    RELATIONS    BETWEEN    THE  PARTS  OF  THE 

INDIVIDUAL. 

(See  Owen,  On  the  Archetype  and  Homologies  of  the  Vertebrate  Skeleton, 
London,  1848,  pp.  5-8;  Gegenijaur,  Comparative  Anatomy,  pp.  58-65; 
Bateson,  Materials  for  the  Study  of  Variation,  pp.  17-22,  87-90,  475-483.) 

Physiological  similarity  — Analogy. 
Morphological  similarity  — Homology. 
Arrangement  of  homologous  parts. 
Symmetry  and  merism. 


8 

Major  and  minor  symmetries. 

Distinction  between  linear  series  and  bilateral  sym- 
metry. 

Comparison  with  radial  symmetry. 

Abnormal  secondary  symmetry. 

Kinds  of  Homology. 

1.  General. 

a.  Bilateral  symmetry,  homotypy. 

b.  Serial  homology,  homodynamy. 
r.    Radial  homology. 

2.  Special  homology. 

a.  Complete. 

b.  Incomplete. 


VI.    THE    ORIGIN    OF    THE    INDIVIDUAL. 

i^Mo  rpJiogciiesis. ) 

I.    Abiogenesis  vs.  Biogenesis. 

(See  Huxley,  Spontaneous  Generation,  Lay  Sermons,  etc.,  p. 
345,  also  Collected  Essays,  Vol.  8,  pp.  229-271  ;  CuEYNE, 
Antiseptic  Surgery,  chapters  8  to  11.) 

Spontaneous  generation  generally  accepted,  Aris- 
totle (384-321  B.  c.) ;  Helmont  (1577-1644); 
Harvey  (i  578-1657)  (Works,  p.  427). 

Spontaneous  generation  of  the  higher  animals  dis- 
proved, Redi  (1 664- 1 690). 

Spontaneous  generation  advanced  on  philosophic 
grounds,  Buffon  (i 707-1 788),  and  Needham. 

Experiments  of  Spallanz.ani,  Sciiultze,  Schwann, 
SciiROEDER,  and  Dusch. 

Heterogenesis,  Pouchet  (1859). 


Method   of    pure   cultures,  Theory  of  specific  organ- 
isms, Pasteur  {i86i). 

Spontaneous  generation  again,  Bastian  {1872). 

Biogenesis   firmly  established,    Cheyne,   Cohn,   Rob- 
erts, Tyndall,  etc. 

2.    Evolution  vs.  Epigenesis. 

(See    article    Embryology,   Encyclopaedia    Britannica,  9th    Ed.; 
Hertwig,  Text-book  of  Embryology,  pp.  23-27.) 

Aristotle,  observations  on  the  chick  (History  of 
Animals,  p.  J  42). 

Harvey  (165 i).      (See  his  works  pp.  228,  336,  457.) 

"  Omne  vivum  ex  ovo." 

"  Epigenesis." 
Malpighi  (1672)    (Appendix  to  Anatome  Plantarum). 

"  Praedelineation." 

Bonnet  and  Haller  (1742-1768),  "Evolution." 
(See  Whitman,  Woods  HoU  Lectures,  1S94,  p.  225.) 

Discovery  of  spermatozoa,  Hamm  and    Leeuwenhoek 

(1677). 

Ovists  (Schwammerdam,  Malpighi,  Haller,  Bon- 
net, Spallanzani)  vs.  Animalculists  (Leeuwen- 
hoek, Hartsoker,  Dalenpatius). 

Casper  Friedrich  Wolff  (1759),  Epigenesis  re- 
vived. 

Christian  Pander  (18 17),  The  three  germ-layers. 

Carl  Ernst  von  Baer  (18 19-]-). 

The  mammalian  ovum  discovered. 
Epigenesis  established. 
Modern  embryology  founded. 


lO 

3.    The  Advance  of  Embryology. 

The  egg  a  cell,  Sch\vaNx\  (1838). 

Cell-lineage    traced    to    the    egg,    Reichert    (1840), 
KoLLiKER,  and  Virchow. 

Function   of  spermatozoa,  experiments  of  Kolliker 
and  Reichert. 

The   spermatozoon  a  cell,  K   lliker,  La  Vallette, 
and  Flemmix(;. 

Fusion  of  the  nuclei  in  fertilization,   Oscar  Hertwig 

The   same    process    in   plants,  Strasijurger    (1884); 
GuiGNARD  (1 89 1).      (See  Amer.  Nat.,  1892,  p.  424.) 


4.    Modern  Theories  of  Fkktilizatiox. 

Two-fold  effect  of  fertilization. 

Parthenogenesis. 

Normal  and  parthenogenetic  eggs  compared. 

Weismann's  Theory. 

MiNOT  (Embryology,  p.  yy). 

Geddes  and  Thomson  (Evolution  of  Sex,  p.  183). 

Composition  of  male  and  female  nuclei. 

AuERBACH  ( 1 89 1 ).      (See  Amer.  Nat.,  1 892,  p.  624.) 
Watase'   (Jour.  Morph.  Vol.  6,  p.  482). 

The  bearer  of  heredity  ? 

Theories  of  Hertwhv,  Weismann. 

BovARi's  experiments  (Amer.  Nat.,  Vol.  27,  p.  222). 

Seeliger's   criticism  (Jour.  Royal  Mic.  Soc,  1895, 

p.  318).     See  also  Amer.  Nat.,  Vol.  29,  p.  286. 
Observations    of    Fol,    Guignard,    and    Conklin. 

(Woods  Holl  Lectures  1893,  p.  15). 


1 1 


Observations  of  Wheeler,  Mead,  Wilson  and 
Mathews,  and  Bovarl  (Jour.  Morph.,  Vol.  lo; 
and  Jour.  Royal  Mic.  Soc,  1895,  p.  433  ;  Wil- 
son, Atlas  of  the  fertilization  and  karyokinesis  of 
the  ovum,  1895). 


5.    Phases  of  Ontogeny. 

i.    /;/  iinicclhilaj'  organisvis  (See  Hatschek,  Zoologie, 
pp.  56-60). 

Simple  fission. 

Budding  and  development. 

ii.  In  multicellular  organisms  (See  Hatschek,  Zoolo- 
gie, pp.  207-226). 

The  cycle  from  egg  to  ^%g. 

Metamorphosis. 

Hermaphroditism. 

Sexual  dimorphism. 

Secondary  sexual  characters. 

Polymorphism  : 

Correlated  with  division  of  labor. 
Associated  with  change  of  season. 

Regeneration  of  lost  parts. 

Fission  followed  by  regeneration. 

Fission  preceded  by  regeneration. 

Budding. 

Alteration  of  generations,  Chamisso. 

a.  By  fission. 

b.  By  budding. 

c.  By  parthenogenesis. 

d.  By  heterogeny. 


12 


6.    Reaction  of  the  Individual  to  its  Environment. 

Use  and  disuse. 
Heliotropism  and  geotropism. 

Chemotropism. 
Thigmotropism. 
Temperature  and  color. 

Summer  and  winter  coats. 

Seasonal  dimorphism. 

Experiments  of  Weismann  (Studies  in  the  The- 
ory of  Descent,  Vol.  i,  p.  i  and  p.  126),  and 
of  Edwards  (Butterflies  of  North  America, 
Vol.  I,  pp.  7-16). 

Summer  form  changed  to  winter  form  by  cold. 

Winter  form  not  changed  by  heat. 

Poulton's  experiments. 

The  determination  of  sex. 

(See  Geddes  and  Thomson,  Evolution  of  Sex,  pp.  32-54.) 

Mrs.  Treat's  experiments  on  caterpillars. 

Young's  "  "  tadpoles. 

GiROu's  "  "  sheep. 

The  Aphides. 

The  honey-bee. 

Maupas's  experiments  on  Hydatina. 

(See  Watase,  Journ.  Morph.  Vol.  6,  p.  483.) 
Summary. 
Individual  acclimatization. 
Reactions  following  change  of  medium. 

Experiment  on  frog  tadpoles. 
Amblystoma  tigrinum. 
Experiments  of  Schmankewitsch. 

Artemia  salina. 

Artemia  milhausenii. 

Branchinecta  schaefferi. 


13 

Experiments  of  Herbst  on  larvae  of  sea-urchins. 
The  potassium  larva. 
The  lithium  larva. 

Reactions  following  mutilation. 

Healing. 

Regeneration  of  lost  parts. 

Polarization. 

Heteromorphosis    (Loeb,  Woods    Holl    Lect. 

1893)- 
Effects  following  removal  of  testes  and  ovaries. 

Comparison    with   effect   of  excision   of  thyroid 

gland. 


7.    Heredity  and  Variation. 

i.   General  Co7isiderations. 

Meaning  of  heredity. 

Prevalence  of  variation. 

Distinction  between  racial  and  individual  variation. 

Distinction  between   acquired   and  congenital   varia- 
tions. 

Importance  of  congenital  variations  as  a  measure  of 
heredity. 

Distinction    between   continuous    and    discontinuous 
variation.     Sports. 

Distinction    between    substantive,   meristic,   and   ho- 
mceotic  variation. 

ii.   Phenomena  of  Individual  ]^ariatio7i. 

(Darwin,  Animals  and  Plants  under  Domestication  ;  Darwin, 
Descent  of  man;  Galton,  Natural  Inheritance;  Bateson, 
Materials  for  the  Study  of  Variation;  Weismann,  Studies  in 
the  Theory  of  Descent;  Weismann,  Essays  upon  Heredity; 
Ellis,  Man  and  Woman.) 


14 

a.   Slight,  or  continuous  variations. 

Variation  of  stature  among  brothers. 
Variation  of  children  of  like  parents. 
Galton's  observations  on  sweet  peas. 

/;.   Saltatory,  or  discontinuous  variations. 
Substantive. 
Meristic. 
Homoeotic. 
Monstrosities. 
Tumors, 

c.  The  correlation  of  variations. 

(See  Darwin,  Animals  and  Plants  under  Domestication,  Vol.  2, 
pp.  311-332;  Brooks,  Heredity,  p.  157.) 

Correlation  between  associated  parts. 
Correlation  between  homologous  parts. 
Darwin's  examples. 

Monstrosities. 

Varieties  of  pigeons. 

Galton  on  finger  prints. 

Bateson,  bilateral  symmetry  in  variation. 

Thompson  on  correlation  in  Palaemon. 

Anomalous  correlations. 

Tufts    of    feathers    and    perforations   of    the 
skull. 

Color  and  constitutional  peculiarities. 
Symmetry  in  monstrosity,  Bateson. 


15 

d.  The  larval  and   adult  forms  of  an   individual   may- 
vary  independently  of  each  other. 

Larva,  pupa,  and  imago  of  Lepidoptera. 

(Weismann,  Studies  in  the  Theory  of  Descent,  Vol.  2,  pp.  404- 
407,  and  pp.  416-419.) 


e.  Variation   associated   with   changed    conditions   of 
life. 


Acclimatisation. 

(vSee  Darwin,  Animals  and  Plants  under  Domestication,  Vol.  2, 
p.  295;  and  Wallace,  Darwinism,  p.  94.) 

Dogs  and  sheep  in  India. 

Geese  in  Bogota. 

Wheat,  etc. 

Greyhounds  in  Mexico  (Brooks  p.  151.) 

Domestication. 

(Darwin,  1.  c,  Vol.  2,  p.  249-252.) 

Variation  following  cultivation  of  plants. 
Turkeys  reared  from  eggs  of  wild  ones. 
Wild  ducks. 

Effect  not  direct,  but  on  subsequent  genera- 
tions. 
Bateson's  objections. 

f.   Sexually  produced  organisms  the  more  variable. 
(Brooks,  Heredity,  p.  143.) 

The  sweet  orange  in  Italy. 

Bud   variation    (Darwin,  Vol.  i,  pp.    361    and 

389). 
Contrast  with  seminal  variation. 

(T.   Males  more  variable  than  females. 


A 


(Darwin,  Descent  of  Man,   Vol.   i,  p.  266;  Brooks,  p.   160; 
Ellis,  Man  and  Woman,  p.  35S.) 


i6 

iii.    TJic  PJicnomaia  of  Heredity. 

(See  Darwin,  Animals  and  Plants  under  Domestication,  Chap- 
ters XII,  XIII,  and  XIV;  Galton,  Natural  Inheritance; 
Delac.e,  Heredite,  pp.  186-260.) 

The  force  of  heredity. 

Pedigrees  of  domestic  animals. 
Inheritance  of  saltatory  variations. 
Variability    itself    a    variation    that    is    often 
strongly  inherited. 

Capriciousness  of  heredity. 

Non-inheritance  due  to  opposing  conditions. 

Fixedness   of    character   apparently  not   due  to 
antiquity  of  inheritance. 

Inheritance  at  corresponding  periods  of  life. 

Particulate  inheritance  (Galton,  p.  7). 
Heritages  that  blend. 

Stature  :  (Galton,  p.  83). 

Not  affected  by  marriage  selection. 

Not  affected  by  diversity  of  parents. 

"  The  mid-parent." 

"Mid-stature  of  population,"  about  68.4 
inches  =  /'. 

Filial  regression  (pp.  95-97). 

Mlial  deviation  from  /-*  :  to  the  mid-pa- 
rental deviation   :  :  2:3. 

On  the  other  hand,  —  Mid-parental  devi- 
ation from  /"* :  Filial  deviation  :  :  i  :  3 
(See  Table  1 1 ). 

Galton's  law  of  regression  :  "  The  devia- 
tion of  sons  from  /'  is  on  the  average 
equal  to  1-3  of  the  deviation  of  the  pa- 
rent from  /'  and  in  the  same  direction." 
(When  in  one  parent  D  =  o.) 


17 

Heritages  that  are  mutually  exclusive. 

Eye-colors  :  (Galton,  pp.  1 38-1 53  and  212- 
218.) 

Division  into  light  and  dark. 
Distribution  in  the  family. 

When  F  has  peculiarity  D,  his  sons  (S)  will 
have  1-3  D,  each  of  his  parents  (G)  will  have 
1-3  D,  each  of  his  grandparents  (Gg)  will 
have  1-9  D,  etc. 

Inheritance  as  limited  to  sex. 

Prepotency. 

Reversion,  or  Atavism  : 

In  pure  breeds. 

In  crossed  varieties  and  species. 

Bud  reversion. 

In  different  parts  of  the  same  animal. 

Latent  characters. 

Appearance    in  the    children    of  characters  not 
found  in  either  parent. 

Kinds  of  characters  that  may  be  transmitted. 

Telegony. 

iv.    Theories  of  Heredity  and  Development. 

(Lloyd  Morgan,  Animal  Life  and  Intelligence,  pp.  130-176; 
Thomson,  The  History  and  Theory  of  Heredity,  Proc.  Royai 
Society,  Edinburgh,  1889,  Vol.  16,  pp.  91-116;  Darwin,  Ani- 
mals and  Plants  under  Domestication,  2d.  Ed.,  Vol.  2,  pp. 
349-399;  Brooks,  Heredity;  Weismann,  Essays  upon  He- 
redity; Weismann,  The  Germ-plasm;  Romanes,  An  E.xamin- 
ation  of  Weismannism ;  O.  Hertwig,  The  Cell,  1S95 ;  O. 
Hertwig,  Zeit-  und  Streitfragen  der  Biologic,  L  Prjiformation 
oder  Epigenese?  also  trans,  under  title.  The  Biological  Prob- 
lem of  To-Day;  Driesch,  Analytische  Theorie  der  Organ- 
ischen  Entwicklung,  Leipzig,  1894;  Delage,  Heredite,  Paris, 
1895,  pp.  403-813;  Wilson,  The  Cell  in  Development  and 
Inheritance,  1896.) 

Requirements  of  a  theory  of  heredity. 

A   theory  of  heredity  must   be  also   a  theory  of 
development. 


i8 

It  must  include  a  theory  of  variation. 

It  involves  a  conception   of  the  essential   struc- 
ture of  living  matter. 

Principles  invoked  to  account  for  heredity. 

The  theories  in  detail : — 

Democritus  (b.  between  494  and  460  b.  c). 

Seed  of  animals  elaborated  by  contributions  from 
all  parts  of  the  body. 

Aristotle  (384-321  r.  c).     The  formative  influence 
of  the  soul. 

Van  Helmont  (i  577-1644).     Transmission  of  spir- 
itual characters. 

Blumenbach  (i 752-1 840),  Needham,  and  others. 
Nisus  foi'mativus,  vital  force,  etc, 

Descartes  (1662). 

First  attempt  at  a  mechanical  explanation  of  de- 
velopment. 

Bonnet  (1720-1793).     Preformation  and  "  Emboite- 
ment." 

BuFFON  (1749- 1 804). 

Organic    and    inorganic    matter    essentially  dif- 
ferent. 

Organic  molecules. 

Spermatozoa  not  concerned  in  reproduction. 

Conflict  of  maternal  and  paternal  molecules. 

Owen  (1849). 

Continuity  of  germ  cells.      (Afterwards  denied.) 


19 

Spencer  (1864). 

(Principles  of  Biology,  Vol.  i,  pp.  179-183  and  209-292.) 

Theory  of  heredity  part  of  philosophical  system 
of  cosmic  evolution. 

Polarity. 

Physiological  unites. 

Germ  cells  collections  of  physiological  unites. 

Any  force  affecting  a  part  affects  the  whole. 

Acquired  characters  inherited. 

Darwin  (1868).      Provisional  hypothesis   of  Pangen- 
esis. 

Inheritance  due  to  gemmules. 

Variation  due  to  latent  gemmules,  to  rearrange- 
ment  of  gemmules,  or  to  the  inheritance  of 
the  direct  effects  of  the  environment  and  of 
use  or  disuse. 

Galton  (1876). 

Pangenesis  doubtful. 

Transmission  of  acquired  characters  doubtful, 

Continuity  of  the  "Stirp." 

His  (1875). 

Differentiation  of  areas. 

Unequal  growth. 

Comparison  with  waves  formed  in  liquids. 

Haeckel  (1876). 

"  Perigenesis  of  the  plastidules." 
Heredity  is  memory. 


20 


Jager  (1879). 

Continuity  of  the  germ-protoplasm  which  receives 
flavor-  and  odor-substances  from  the  body- 
cells. 

Brooks  (1876,  1883).     Modified  theory  of  Pangenesis. 

Continuity  of  germ-cells. 

Gemmules  given  off  by  body-cells  only  under 
unfavorable  conditions. 

Sperm-cells  especially  modified  to  collect  gem- 
mules. 

Gemmules  cause  variation  in  the  corresponding 
part  of  the  offspring. 

"  The  occurrence  of  a  variation  is  due  to  the  di- 
rect action  of  external  conditions,  but  its  pre- 
cise character  is  not." 

"  The  structure  of  the  adult  is  latent  in  the  egg," 
Evolution. 

Evidence. 

NussBAUM  (1880-1887). 

Continuity  of  germ-cells. 

Nageli  (1884).     Theory  of  the  Idioplasm. 
Formation  of  "  Micellae." 
"Idioplasma"  and  "  Niihrplasma." 
Net-work  of  idioplasm. 
Micellar  threads,  one  for  each  character. 
Their  union  to  form  idioplasmic  cords. 
Elementary  and  complex  characters. 
Same  elements  in  all  jjarts  of  the  idioplasm. 
Germ-cells. 


21 

Sexual  reproduction. 
The  idioplasm  in  heredity. 
Variation. 
The  nucleus  as  the  bearer  of  heredity. 
O.  Hertwig  (1875,  1884,  1886). 
Strasburger  (1884). 
kolliker  (1886). 

BOVARI  (1886). 

E.  B.  Wilson  (1895). 

Roux  (1881,  1885+). 

Struggle  of  the  elements  within  the  cell. 

Struggle  of  the  cells. 

Morphogenic  effect  of  functional  stimuli. 

Heredity  due  to  transmission  of  chemical  com- 
position. 

The  mosaic  theory. 

Weismann.      Earlier  theories  (1883,  1885,  1886,  and 
1887). 

All  inheritance  of  acquired  characters  denied. 

Theory  of  gemmules  unnecessary  and  improbable. 

Continuity  of  germ-cells  disproved. 

Germ-tracts. 

Position  of  the  idioplasm  in  the  nucleus. 

Distinction    between    germ  idioplasm,  or  gcrtii- 
plasin,  and  somatic  idioplasm. 

Improbability  of  the   backward  development   of 
somatic  idioplasm  into  germ-plasm. 

Continuity  of  the  germ-plasm. 


22 


Development  a  process  of  epigenesis. 

Variation  due  to  the  direct  effect  of  external 
conditions  upon  the  germ  (1883). 

Variation  in  higher  organisms  due  principally 
to  the  mingling  of  diverse  germ-plasms  in  sex- 
ual reproduction  (1886). 

First   polar  spindle  removes  the   ovogenic  idio- 
■    plasm  (1887). 

Second  polar  spindle  removes  half  of  the  ances- 
tral germ-plasms. 

De  Vries  (1889).     Intracellular  pangenesis. 

Pangenes  (gemmulcs). 

Reserve  pangenes  in  nucleus,  active  pangenes  in 
cytoplasm. 

Protoplasmic  net-work  and  inheritance  of  ac- 
quired characters  denied. 

Variation  due  to,  —  modification,  change  in  pro- 
portion, alteration  in  arragement,  or  unequal 
division,  of  pangenes  in  germ-cells. 

Continuity  of  germ-plasm. 

Independence  of  hereditary  characters  and  the 
necessity  for  separate  factors. 

Control  of  the  cell  by  the  nucleus  and  the  ne- 
cessity for  material  particles. 

Weismann.     Later  theory  (189 1,  1893). 

Development  a  process  of  evolution. 

Continuity  of  the  germ-plasm. 

The  mingling  of  gerni-i)lasms  in  sexual  repro- 
duction, A  nipli  iiii  iv  IS. 

Each  ancestral  germ-i^lasni  a  distinct  unite,  the 
id 


23 

Each  chromosome,  or  idant,  composed  of  many 
ids. 

Each  id  composed  of  smaller  unites,  the  deter- 
viiuauts,  one  for  each  independently  variable 
part  of  the  organism. 

Each  determinant  composed  of  the  ultimate 
unites  of  living  matter,  biopJiors. 

Control  of  the  cell  by  the  nucleus  and  the  ne- 
cessity for  biophors. 

The  id  in  ontogeny. 

The  division  of  the  id  of  germ-plasm   into 

an  active  id  of  somatic  idioplasm  and  an 

id  of  reserve  germ-plasm. 
Growth   and    multiplication   by   division   of 

the  determinants  and  ids. 
The  qualitative  and  quantitative  divisions  of 

the  active  ids. 
The  migration  of  the  biophors  of  the  active 

determinants  from  the   nucleus  into   the 

cell-body. 
Fate  of  the  reserve  germ-plasm. 

The  effect  of  amphimixis. 

The  homologous  determinants  in  the  ids  may 
be  homodynamous  or  heterodynamous. 

Hence  a  possible  struggle  of  the  ids  in 
ontogeny. 

Significance  of  the  polar  bodies. 

The  maturation  of  the  Qgg  and  the  forma- 
tion of  the  spermatozoon  preceded  by  the 
doubling  of  the  idants  and  ids. 

The  first  subsequent  division  (first  polar 
spindle)  reduces  this  number  by  half. 

The  second  division  (second  polar  spindle  in 
the  Q^g^  reduces  the  remainder  by  half. 

Advantage  of  two  reducing  divisions  in  in- 
creasing variability. 

Origin  of  variations. 


24 

Reversion  due,  in  hybrids,  to  the  effect  of  the 
reducing  divisions  and  favorable  combinations  ; 
in  pure  races,  to  ids  becoming  dominant  after 
a  latent  period. 

Sexual  dimorphism  due  to  doubling  of  the  de- 
terminants for  the  sexual  characters. 

Regeneration  of  lost  parts  due  to  special  deter- 
minants in  the  germ-plasm. 

Reproduction  by  fission  the  same  process  carried 
farther. 

Budding  due  to  a  doubling  of  the  ids  of  germ- 
plasm. 

Alternations  of  generations  due  to  the  presence 
of  two  kinds  of  germ-plasm  in  each  reproduc- 
tive cell. 


Weismannism  criticised. 

Points  probably  well  founded  :  — 

Inheritance  of  acquired  characters  unproved. 
Idioplasm     contained    in     chromatin    of    the 

nucleus. 
Continuity   of  idioplasm     between  successive 

generations. 
The  characters   of   the  offspring  of  germinal 
and  not  of  somatic  origin. 

Main  questions  now  under  discussion  :  — 

Is  there  a  real  difference  between  germ-plasm 

and  somatic  idioplasm  ? 
Is  development  a  process  of  evolution   or  of 

epigencsis  .'' 
May  qualitative  cell-division  occur  ? 
How  far  is  ontogeny  due  to  the   structure    of 

the  idioplasm  and  how  far  to  environment  ? 
Is  VVeismann's  interpretation  of  the  reducing 
divisions  correct  ? 


25 

Is   Amphimixis  sufficient   to  account  for   the 
main  facts  of  variation  ? 

The  mosaic  theory  of  development  as   evidence 
for  qualitative  divisions. 

Experiments  of  Roux  on  Frogs'  eggs. 

Formation  of  half  embryos. 

Post  generation. 

Roux's  explanation. 
Chambry's  experiments  on  eggs  of  Ascidia  as- 

pera. 
E.  B.  Wilson  on  the  cell  lineage  of  Nereis. 

Evidence  against  qualitative  divisions. 

1.  In  normal  development. 

a.  Lack    of    correspondence    between    first 

cleavage  plane  and  any  plane  of  adult 

body. 
Morgan  on  frogs  and  teleosts. 
Miss  Clapp  on  toad  fish. 

b.  Variations  in  cleavage. 

H.  V.  Wilson  on  sea-bass. 

Jordan  on  Amphibia. 

E.  B.  Wilson  on  Amphioxus. 

Three  types  with  all  grades  between. 

Result  normal  embryos. 

2.  Experimental  evidence. 

a.  Effects  of  pressure. 

Hertwig,  pressure  on  frogs'  eggs,  — 
Abnormal  cleavage  but  normal  embryos. 

Morgan,   removal  of   yolk  from   c^^  of 
Fnndiilns,  — 
Abnormal  cleavage  but  normal  embryos. 

b.  Isolation  of  blastomeres. 

Driesch,  eggs  of  Echinus  in  2-cell  stage. 
Result :  half  blastulas,  becoming  com- 
plete dwarf  blastulas,  gastrulas  and 
plutei. 

LoEB,  on  eggs  of  sea-urchins. 


26 


Morgan,  one  blastomerc  killed  in  2-cell 
stage  of  Fiimiiilus.  Result  perfect  em- 
bryos 2-3  normal  size. 

Wilson  on  eggs  of  Ampliioxus. 

2-cell  stage,  perfect  blastulas  gastrulas 

and  nearly  perfect  larvae  1-2  size. 
4-cell  stage,  less  perfect  larvae  1-4  size. 
8-cell  stage,  imperfect  blastulas. 

Wilson's  arguments  against  qualitative 
divisions  and  the  theory  of  determi- 
nants, and  in  favor  of  epigenesis. 

c.  Effects  of  abnormal  stimuli  on  develop- 
ment. 
Herbst,   Formative  stimuli  in  ontogeny. 

Evidence    against    distinction     between     germ- 
plasm  and  somatic  idioplasm  afforded  by  the 
phenomena  of  regeneration  and  budding. 
G.  Wolff,  regeneration  of  the  lens  in  Trito?i. 

Evidence  against    Weismann's  view  of    the  re- 
ducing divisions. 

Strasburger,    different    number   of   chromo- 
somes in  sexual  and  asexual  generation  of 
plants. 
Corresponding  phenomena  in  animals. 

Brooks's  criticism  of  amphimixis  as  a  cause  of 
variation. 

The  extermination  of  families. 

O.  Hertwig  (1892,  1894). 

The  idioblasts. 

Their  fusion  during  fertilization. 

The  structure  of   the  adult  only  indirectly  the 
result  of  the  structure  of  the  idioplasm  of  the 

Every  cell-division  quantitative  only. 

Development  a  process  of  epigenesis. 

Each  stage  in  ontogeny  determines  only  the  next 
stage. 


27 

The  differentiation  of  the  cell,  a  function  of  its 
position. 

Comparison  of  organic  development  with  devel- 
opment of  the  state. 

VII.     WHAT  IS  MEANT  BY  "SPECIES." 

(Jevons,  The  Principles  of  Science,  pp.  698-734  ;  Article  Zoology  in  En- 
cyclopaedia Britannica,  9th  Ed. ;  Carus,  Geschichte  der  Zoologie,  p.  434  et 
seq.\   Wallace,  Darwinism,  pp.  1-2.) 

The  discontinuity  of  living  forms. 

Genus  and  Species  in  Logic. 

The  terms  used  in  this  sense  by  the  early  naturahsts. 

Organic  species  defined  by  John  Ray  (1686). 

LlNN^US  (1753). 

Best  definition  by  de  Condolle. 

SWAINSON. 

Variety,  species,  and  genus  contrasted. 
Species  the  unite  group. 
The  next  problem. 

VIII.     THE  ORIGIN  OF  SPECIES. 
( Phylogenesis) 

I.     Special  Creation  vs.  Organic  Evolution. 
i.      Special  Great io7i. 

(John  Ray,  The  Wisdom  of  God  manifested  in  the  Works  of 
the  Creation ;  John  Ray,  Three  PhysicoTheological  Dis- 
courses; H.ECKEL,  The  History  of  Creation,  Vol.  i,pp- 37-71  ; 
Bridgewater  Treatises,  Vol.  i,  pp.  17-55,  ^"<^  Vol.  3,  pp.  1-32 
et  set].;  L.  Agassiz,  Essay  on  Classification,  Contributions  to 
the  Natural  History  of  the  United  States,  Vol.  i,  pp.  3-232; 
The  Duke  of  Argyle,  The  Reign  of  Law,  pp.  20S-273.) 

Antiquity  of  the  theory.  Book  of  Genesis,  i  and  ii. 


28 

John  Ray  (1691).     Design  in  Nature. 

LiNN/EUS.  Each  species  descended  from  a  single 
pair. 

CuviER.  The  immutability  of  species  and  the  doctrine 
of  catastrophes. 

Agassiz  {1857).  Each  species  created  with  full  num- 
bers and  in  position  where  discovered.  His  ideal- 
istic view  of  species.  ^ 

The  Duke  of  Argyle  (1866).  Creation  by  law  is 
nothing  but  the  reign  of  an  intelligent  and  pur- 
poseful Creative  Force.  Criticisms  of  Darwin  (See 
below). 

ii.      TJie  Unity  of  Nature  as  shown  by  Geology. 

(Lyell,  Principles  of  Geology,  1830,  ist.  Amer.  Ed.  1S37  ;  Cu- 
viER,  Discours  sur  les  Revolutions  de  la  Surface  du  Globe, 
1825,  see  also  trans.,  entitled  Theory  of  the  Earth.) 

The  shortness  of  the  earth's  history  as  calculated 
from  the  Jewish  scriptures. 

Date  of  creation  4004  b.  c,  according  to  Archbishop 
Usher. 

Early  geologists, — catastrophes,  changes  in  the  laws 
of  Nature. 

HuTTON  (1788).  No  changes  in  natural  laws,  catas- 
trophes a  part  of  the  permanent  order  of  Nature, 

Sir  Charles  Lyell  (1830).  The  doctrine  of  catas- 
trophes without  scientific  basis. 

All    geological    changes   may   be    explained    by 
forces  now  at  work. 

The  establishment  of  an  orderly  course  in  the  devel- 
oi)ment  of  the  earth's  surface  makes  possible  a 
theory  of  evolution  of  organic  forms. 


iii.      The  Meanings  of  the  luord  "  Evolution. 


29 

In  philosophy  :  — 

"  A  change  from  an  indefinite,  incoherent  homo- 
geneity, to  a  definite,  coherent  heterogeneity, 
through  continuous  differentiation  and  integra- 
tion."    Spencer  (First  Principles). 

In  biology  :  — 

As  applied  to  the  origin  of  the  individual,  — 

"  The    becoming    perceptible    of    preexisting 
latent  diversities,"  Roux.    (See  above,  VI,  2.) 

As  applied  to  the  origin  of  species,  — 

Descent   with   modification   from    preexisting 
species. 

The  theory  of  organic  evolution  stated. 

iv.    The  Evidence  of  Organic  Evolution. 

(Romanes,  Darwin  and  after  Darwin,  Vol.  i,pp.  23-248;  Spen- 
cer, Principles  of  Biology,  Vol.  I,  pp.  346-401  ;  Wallace, 
On  the  Law  which  has  Regulated  the  Introduction  of  New 
Species,  Annals  and  Magazine  of  Natural  History,  Sept.  1855, 
(Ser.  2),  Vol.  16,  p.  184;  Fritz  Muller,  Fiir  Darwin,  1864, 
see  also  Trans.,  Facts  and  Arguments  for  Darwin,  1869 ; 
Wiedersheim,  Structure  of  Man,  1895.) 

1.  The  evidence  from  Classification. 

Failure  of  the  linear  arrangement. 

True  relations  shown  only  by  tree-like  arrange- 
ment, e.  g.  Scyphomedusae. 

This  arrangement  best  explained  by  evolution. 

Gradations  of  species  into  one  another. 

Calcareous  sponges. 

2.  The  evidence  from  Geographical  Distribution. 

The  continuous  distribution  of  a  species. 

Distribution  of  the  species  in  a  genus  often  dis- 
continuous. 


30 

Similarity  between  the  distribution  of  species  and 
varieties.     Cya7iura  stclleri. 

"  Present  distribution  cannot  be  accounted  for  by 
difference  in  physical  conditions. 

Importance  of  barriers  (Darwin,  pp.  322-329). 

Affinity  of  productions  of  the  same  continent. 

"  Centres  of  Creation." 

Some  facts  accounted  for  by  the  past  history  of 
the  earth  (See  Wallace,  pp.  338-355). 

Facts  to  be  explained. 
Conditions  of  distribution. 
Permanence  of  oceans. 
Oceanic  and  continental  areas. 
Madagascar  and  New  Zealand. 
The  thousand  fathom  line. 
The  distribution  of  Marsupials. 
The  distribution  of  Tapirs. 
Powers  of  dispersal. 

3.  The  evidence  from  Palaeontology. 

(Romanes,  1892,  pp.  156-203.) 

Incompleteness  of  the  record. 

Difficulty  of  preservation. 

Full   history   of   specific   changes  not  to   be  ex- 
pected. 

Palaeontology  furnishes  no  proof  against  evolu- 
tion. 

Le  Conte's  diagram  of  the  succession  of  animals. 

Missing  links. 

The  development  of  horns. 

The  tails  of  fishes. 

The  tails  of  birds. 

Arclueoptcrix. 


31 

Mammalian  limbs. 

Baptanodon. 

Chelydra. 

Evolution  of  ungulate  limb, 

Zygapophyses, 

Teeth. 

The  brain. 

Evolution  of  Planorbis  at  Steinheim, 

Strombiis  in  Florida. 

4.  The  evidence  from  Embryology. 

(Romanes,  pp,  147-154.) 

Recapitulation   of  geological  stages  in  ontogeny. 

Antlers  of  the  stag. 

Tails  of  fishes, 

This  also  a  recapitulation  of  the  classification 
of  existing  forms. 

Law  that  higher  forms  pass  through  ontogenetic 
stages  corresponding  to  lower  forms  well  shown 
by  Scyphomedusae. 

The  earlier  stages  in  ontogeny  are  common  to 
the  largest  groups  while  the  latter  stages  are 
restricted  to  smaller  and  smaller  groups. 

Egg  not  found  in  Protozoa,  but  is  the  protozoan 
stage  in  all  higher  groups. 

Gastrula  present  in  higher  coelenterates  and  is 
coelenterate  stage  in  all  higher  forms. 

Early  stages  of  echinoderm  larva  of  one  type  for 
whole  group.  This  developes  into  a  different 
type  of  larva  for  each  sub-group. 

The  nauplius  (Korschelt  and  Heider.) 

Special  larval  forms  of  groups  of  Crustacea. 


The  life  history  of  Decapods. 

Appearance  in  embryo  of  oro^ans  only  functional 
in  lower  forms,  e.  g.,  gill-slits  in  vertebrates. 

5.  The  evidence  from  Vestigial  Organs. 
(Romanes,  pp.  65-97  ;  Wiedersheim.) 

Teeth  and  ear  of  foetal  whale. 

Limbs  of  python. 

Wings  of  Apteryx. 

Wings  of  insects  of  Madeira  and  Kerguelen. 

Blind  animals  in  caves. 

Agassiz's  views. 

Universal  occurrence  of  vestigial  organs. 

Man. 

Nictitating  membrane. 

Muscles  of  external  ear. 

Feet  and  hands  of  infants. 

Tail. 

Vermiform  appendix  of  the  coecum. 

Ear. 

Hair. 

Teeth. 

6.  The  evidence  from  Homologies. 

(Romanes,  pp.  50-65.) 

Homology  without  analogy. 
Whales  and  seals. 

Homology  with  analogy. 
The  wings  of  vertebrates. 

Analogy  without  homology. 

Contrast  between  eye  of  octopus  and  of  fish. 

Structure   of    Dinornis  opposed  to  the    idea  of 
special  creation  of  types. 

The  cocoanut  crab  of  Keeling  Island. 


33 

7-   Summary  of  the  evidence. 

All  points  towards  evolution,  no  evidence  against 
it. 

Evidence  largely  circumstantial. 

The  succession  of  horse-like  forms,  the  Planorbis 
shells  of  Steinheim,  and  similar  cases  afford 
direct  evidence. 


2.     Theories  of  Organic  Evolution. 

(OsBORN,  From  the  Greeks  to  Darwin  ;  Haeckel,  The  History 
of  Creation,  Vol.  i,  pp.  70-174;  Huxley,  Article  Evolution, 
Encyclopaedia  Britannica,  9th  Ed.,  Vol.  8,  pp.  741-754  ;  Pack- 
ard, Introduction,  Riverside  Natural  History,  Vol.  i,  pp. 
1-lxii;  Darwin,  Autobiography,  Life  and  Letters,  Vol.  i,  pp. 
26-107  ;  Wallace,  Natural  Selection  and  Tropical  Nature, 
pp.  3-33  and  450-475;  Marshall,  Lectures  on  the  Darwinian 
Theory,  pp.  1-24,  and  200-228.) 

i.    The  Rise  of  the  Theory  of  Descent. 

The  Greeks. 

Aristotle,  etc. 

The  "  Naturphilosophen." 

Goethe  (1790).    Metamorphosis  of  Plants. 

Theory  of  the  skull. 

Treviranus.     Adaptation. 

Erasmus  Darwin  (1795).     Effects  of  new  condi- 
tions. 

Oken.     Theory  of  the  skull.     "  Urschleim." 

ii.    The  TJieory  of  Direct  Modification. 

Lamarck  (1809).    Habit  and  the  effect  of  use  and 
disuse. 

Geoffrey  St.  Hilaire.    Effects  of  changes  in  exter- 
nal conditions. 

The  debate  of  1830. 


34 
iii.    The  Theory  of  Selection. 

Charles  Darwin. 

The  voyage  in  the  Beagle  (183 1.) 

L yell's  Geology. 

The  fauna  of  the  Pampas. 

The  South  American  affinities  of  the  productions 
of  the  Galapagos  and  their  relations  to  one 
another. 

Malthus  on  population     (1803). 

Alfred  Russell  Wallace. 

The  journey  to  the  Malay  Archipelago. 

The  paper  of  1855  —  "Every  species  has  come 
into  existence  coincident  both  in  space  and 
time  with  a  preexisting,  closely  allied  species." 

The  discovery  of  the  principle  of  the  survival  of 
the  fittest,  February  (1858). 

Darwin  and  Wallace  (1858). 

Origin  of  Species  (1859). 

The  cue  furnished  by  Malthus  and  Franklin 
(1751)- 

The  theory  of  natural  selection  stated. 
The  tendency  to  multiply. 
Heredity  and  variation. 
The  struggle  for  existence. 
The  survival  of  the  fittest. 
The  .struggle  against  enemies. 
The  struggle  against  physical  conditions. 


35 

iv.    Neo-Lamarckians,  vs.  Neo-Daiivmiatts. 
The  inheritance  of  acquired  characters. 
The  all-sufficiency  of  natural  selection. 

3.    The  Factors  of  Organic  Evolution. 
i.     Variation  and  Heredity. 

(Darwin,  Origin  of  Species,  Chap.  V. ;  Brooks,  Heredity,  pp. 
140-165;  Wallace,  Darwinism,  pp.  41-125  ;  Lloyd  Morgan, 
Animal  Life  and  Intelligence,  pp.  61-75;  Bateson,  Materials 
for  the  study  of  Variation  ;  Cope,  Primary  Factors  of  Organic 
Evolution,  pp.  21-73.) 

Variation  and  the  inheritance  of  variations,  the  basis 
of  any  theory  of  evolution. 

a.    Kinds  of  Variation. 

(i)    As  to  distribution. 

Variation  in  the  individual. 

Acquired. 
Congenital. 

Variation  in  the  species,  Racial  Variation. 

Race.     Variety.     Sub-species. 
No  sharp  line  to  be  drawn  between  charac- 
ters of  the  family,  variety,  and  species. 

(2)    As  to  quality. 

Substantive. 

Chemical  composition.     Color.     Size.     Pro- 
■^  portions. 

Meristic. 

Pattern.     Symmetry.     Homoeosis. 

Other  qualities. 

Age  at  which  various  characters  are  acquired. 

Acclimatization. 

Habits. 

Change  of  function. 


36 

Instincts. 

Intelligence. 

Variability. 

(3)    As  to  quantity. 

Moderate  variations. 
Sports. 

Prevalence  and  Extent  of  Variation. 

That  variation  in  wild  species  is  not  infrequent 
nor  always  slight  in  amount  pointed  out  by 
Wallace, 

Foraminifera. 

Sea-Anemones. 

Nerita. 

Cassiopea,  and  other  Medusae. 

Gulick's  studies  on  the  land  shell  of  Oahu. 

Helix  in  France. 

Mollusca  of  Colorado. 

Insects. 

Milne-Edvvards's  measurements  Of  lizards. 

Allen's  measurements  of  birds. 

Variation  in  size  i  ^%  to  20^. 

Each  part  varies  independent  of  the  others. 

Same  result  from  Allen's  measurements  of 
squirrels. 

Also  Llovii  Morgan's  measurements  of  bats' 
wings  (Animal  Life,  pp.  63-75). 

Variation  in  internal  organs. 

Beddard  on  earthworms. 

Skulls  of  orang-utans. 

Skulls  of  wolves  and  bears. 
Batkson  on  the  frequency  of  sports. 


37 

Distribution  of  individual  variations  within  the 
species. 

(i)    The  normal  curves  of  variations. 

Galton's  observations  on  the  English  people. 

{See  Galton,  Natural  Inheritance,  pp.  35-70.) 

The  curve  of  distribution. 

The  curve  of  frequency. 

Relations  of  these  curves. 

The  curve  of  frequency  of  error. 

Mechanical    illustration  of  the   cause  of    the 
curve  of  frequency. 

Weldon's  observations  on  Crangon  and  Car- 
ciniis. 
(Proc.  Roy.  Soc,  Vol.  47,  p.  445,  and  Vol.  54,  p.  318). 

(2)    The  correlation  of  variations. 

(See  Darwin,  Animals  and  Plants  under  Domestication, 
Vol.  2,  pp.  311-332  ;  and  Brooks,  Heredity,  p.  157.) 

Correlation  between  associated  parts. 
Correlation  between  homologous  parts. 

Galton's  function   (See   Galton,   Proc.    Roy. 
Soc,  Vol.  45,  p.  135  ;  Weldon,  /.  c,  Vol. 

55»  P-  234)- 

M^  mean,  Q  =  "probable  error." 

Qa.  =  the  Q  of  organ  A. 

Qb   =  the  Q  of  organ  B. 

V  =z  any  deviation  of  organ  A  from  its  M. 
Xm  =  mean  associated  deviation  of  organ 

B  from  its  M. 
X  =  any  deviation  of  organ  B    from  its  M. 
Ym  =  mean  associated  deviation  of  organ 
A   from  its  M. 

X».  ^   Qb  Y,n  ^Qa 

^=: -— =  r,  a  constant, 

Y  -^Qa       X   -^Q, 


38 

Where  the  variations  in  two  organs  are  per- 
fectly correlated  ^  =  -j-  i . 

Where  two  organs  vary  entirely  independ- 
ently r^  o. 

Value  of  r  the  same  for  any  given  pair  of 
organs  through  )ut  the  species,  Weldon. 

Value  of  ;'  higher  between  homologous  parts 
and  between  adjacent  parts,  than  between 
parts  not  so  related,  Weldon,  Thompson. 

(3)  Parallel  variation. 
Peacocks. 
Nectarines. 

d.    Laws  of  Racial  Variation. 

Specific  characters  more  variable  than  generic. 

"  A  part  developed  in  any  species  in  an  extraor- 
dinary degree  or  manner,  in  comparison  with 
the  same  part  in  allied  species,  tends  to  be 
highly  variable." 

Secondary  sexual  characters. 

Usually  confined  to  males. 

Often  developed  in   an  extraordinary  manner 

or  degree. 
Highly  variable. 

Law  of  "  equable  variation." 

"  The  species  of  the  larger  genera  in  each  coun- 
try vary  more  frequently  than  species  of  the 
smaller  genera." 

'•  Wide  ranging  much  diffused  and  common  spe- 
cies vary  most." 

Crossing  leads  to  variability. 


39 

e.    The  Inheritance  of  Variations. 

The  force  of  heredity. 

Capriciousness  of  heredity. 

(See  above  VI.  6,  iii.) 

Racial  variations  arise  from   the  inheritance  of 
individual  variations. 

ii.    The  Struggle  for  Existence. 

(Benjamin  Franklin,  Observations  concerning  the  Increase  of 
Mankind,  Collected  Works,  Vol.  2,  p.  231  ;  Malthus,  Essay 
on  the  Principle  of  Population,  1803  ;  Darwin,  Origin  of 
Species,  Chap.  Ill;  Wallace,  Darwinism,  pp.  14-40;  Hud- 
son, The  Naturalist  in  La  Plata,  pp.  59-68  ;  Brehm,  From 
North  Pole  to  Equator;  Romanes,  Darwin  and  after  Darwin, 
Vol.  I, pp.  259-270.) 

The  apparent  peace  in  nature. 

Species  on  the  average  contain  a  constant  number  of 
individuals. 

This  in  spite  of  the  tendency  to  increase  in  geometri- 
cal ratio. 

Examples  —  The  elephant. 

Man. 

The  carrion  fly. 

The  common  birds. 
Effect  of  introduction  into  a  free  field. 
Our  common  weeds. 
The  English  sparrow  in  our  country. 
The  horses  and  cattle  of  the  plains. 
Rabbits  in  New  Zealand. 
Hogs  in  Central  America. 
The  tendency  of  plants  to  increase. 
The  great  number  of  seeds  produced. 


40 

European  thistles  on  the  La  Plata. 
Cotton  weed  in  the  tropics. 
White  clover  in  New  Zealand. 
Other  plants  in  New  Zealand. 
Lantana  in  Ceylon. 

The  lack  of  increase  ordinarily,  in  spite  of  the  number 
of  young  produced,  shows  that  there  must  be  a 
large  death  rate. 

The  checks  on  population  and  the  struggle  for  exis- 
tence. 

a.  The  scarcity  of  food. 

The  struggle  for  food  between  individuals  of  the 
same  species. 

Between  different  species. 

Examples  —  The  trees  in  a  forest. 

Darwin's  experiment  with  the  turf. 

The  beech  and  birch  in  Denmark. 

The  water  cress  and  the  willows. 

b.  Enemies. 

Dependence  of  herbivorous  animals  upon  plants 
and  of  carnivorous  animals  upon  herbivorous 
ones. 

The  struggle  to  escape  being  eaten. 

Seeds. 

Absence  of  trees  on  the  Pampas. 

The  struggle  on  the  river  banks. 

Darwin's  example  of  the  game  animals. 

P^ishcs. 

Parasites. 


41 

c.    Unfavorable  climate. 

The  winter  of  1854-55  in  England. 

Recapitulation. 

The  Complexity  of  the  Struggle. 

Hypothetical  example. 

Cats  and  the  crop  of  clover  seed. 

Salmon  and  the  inland  birds. 

Trees  on  the  heath  in  Staffordshire. 
Birth  rate  proportional  to  the  risk  of  destruction. 

Contrast  fishes  and  petrel. 

The  passenger  pigeon. 

iii.    NaUiral  Selection. 

(Darwin,  Origin  of  Species,  Chap.  IV  ;  Wallace,  Darwinism, 
pp.  102-151,  187-267,  and  301-337;  Lloyd  Morgan,  Aninial 
Life  and  Intelligence,  pp.  77-121  ;  Romanes,  Darwin  and  after 
Darwin,  pp.  251-378  ;  Marshall,  Lectures  on  the  Darwinian 
Theory,  pp.  27-52  and  116-172.) 

a.    Evidences  for  the  theory. 

(i)  The  observed  fact  that  the  struggle  for  ex- 
istence leads  to  the  extermination  of  forms 
less  fitted  for  the  struggle  and  thus  makes 
room  for  forms  more  fitted. 

(2)  Not  a  single  structure  or  instinct  in  the  ani- 

mal or  vegetable  kingdom  is  developed 
for  the  exclusive  benefit  of  another  spe- 
cies. 

Apparent  objections  :  — 

Secretion  of  aphides  useful  to  ants. 

Vegetable  galls  of  use  to  insects. 

(3)  The  efficacy  of  artificial  selection. 

(See  Romanes,  figs.  91-107 ;  Marshall,  figs.  1-3 ;  Bailey, 
Plant-Breeding.) 


42 

Apparent  objections :  — 

The  selecting  agent  differs  in  natural  and 
artificial  selection. 

Varieties  produced  by  artificial  selection 
differ  from  true  species  in  not  being 
mutually  infertile. 

b.  Applications  of  the  theory  :  — 

(i)    Adaptations  of  flowers  for  fertilization    by 
insects. 

(2)  Structures     and     movements    of    climbing 

plants. 

(3)  Protective  coloring  in  animals. 

(4)  Warning  colors. 

(5)  Mimicry. 

All  of  these  facts  can  only  be  explained   by 
the  theory  of  natural  selection. 

c.  Criticisms  of  the  theory. 

(i)   Owen:  Figurative  language  explains  nothing. 

(2)  Duke  of    Argyll  :    Natural    selection   can 

produce  nothing, 

(3)  If  some,  why  not  all  species  improved  by  nat- 

ural selection  } 

(4)  Why  have  not  superior  forms  exterminated 

inferior  ones  inhabiting  the  same  locality  } 

All  the  above  objections  arise  from  a 
misunderstanding  of  the  theory. 

(5)  Similar  organs  or  structures  are  met  with  in 

widely  different  groups. 

This  is  true  as  to  analogy,  but  never  as 
to  homology. 

(6)  Beginnings  of  organs  are  useless   and  can- 

not be  selected. 


43 

Some  organs  useful,  however  slightly  de- 
veloped. 

Change  of  function. 

Correlation  of  variations. 

(7)  Difficulty  of  explaining    electric    organ    in 

skate. 

An  isolated  case  that  probably  will  be  ex- 
plained with  increased  knowledge. 

(8)  Uselessness  of  many  specific  characters. 

(9)  Cross  infertility  between  species  cannot  be 

due  to  natural  selection. 

(10)    Free  intercrossing  renders  divergent  evolu- 
tion impossible. 

The  last  three  not  valid  objections  unless 
natural  selection  is  regarded  as  the  sole 
factor  of  organic  evolution. 


iv.    Panmixia  and  the  Reversal  of  Selection. 

(Lloyd  Morgan,  Animal  Life  and  Intelligence,  pp.  189-197; 
Darwin,  Origin  of  Species,  Vol.  i,  pp.  182-183,  and  Vol.  2, 
pp.  255-263,  Amer.  Ed.  pp.  149-151  and  404-410;  Weis- 
MANN,  Retrogressive  Development  in  Nature,  Essays  upon 
Heredity,  Vol.  2,  pp.  3-30;  Romanes  and  Lankester,  Let- 
ters in  Nature,  Vol.  41,  pp.  437-486,  511,  558,  and  584,  and 
Vol.  42,  pp.  5,  52,  and  79;  Romanes,  Darwin  and  after  Dar- 
win, Vol.  2,  pp.  291-306;  Weismann,  Germinal  Selection, 
1896.) 

Dwindling  and  disappearance  of  organs  during  phy- 
logeny. 

Inadequacy  of  the  inheritance  of  the  effects  of  dis- 
use as  an  explanation. 

Loss  of  parts  in  flowers. 

Loss  of  protective  structures  in  animals. 

Loss  of  wings  in  neuter  insects. 


L  I  B  R  A  R  Yi3o| 

—  ,.-^ 

#/      _       V  %/ 


44 

Panmixia,   or   the    cessation  of  selection,    Romanes, 
Weismann. 

The   reduction   from   survival   mean   to   birth 
mean. 

Economy  of  growth  and  reversed  selection,  Darwin. 

Loss  of  carapace  in  parasitic  barnacles. 

Loss  of  wings  in  insular  insects. 
Difficulty  as  to  the  final  disappearance  of  organs. 

Reversed  selection,  Panmixia,  Weismann. 

Failure  of  heredity,  Romanes. 

Germinal  selection,  Weismann. 

v.    Sexual  Selection. 

(Darwin,  The  Descent  of  Man,  Vol.  i,  pp.  245-409,  and  Vol. 
2,  pp.  1-387  ;  Wallace,  Natural  Selection  and  Tropical  Na- 
ture, pp.  338-394;  Wallace,  Darwinism,  pp.  268-300;  Ro- 
manes, Darwin  and  after  Darwin,  Vol.  i,  pp.  284-335  !  Lloyd 
Morgan,  Animal  Life  and  Intelligence,  pp.  197-209;  Brehm, 
North  Pole  to  Equator.) 

a.  The  law  of  battle. 

b.  The  aesthetic  sense  of  birds. 

c.  Courtship. 

Among  birds. 
Among  spiders. 

d.  Ornamental  secondary  sexual  characters  developed 
by  sexual  selection. 

e.  Evidence. 

(1)  These  characters  are  confined  to  the  sex  that 

is  active  in  courtship,  almost  always  the 
male. 

(2)  They  are  as  a  rule  developed  only  at  matur- 

ity and  often   only  during   the   breeding 
season. 


45 

(3)  Are  always  and  only  displayed  in  perfection 

during  courtship. 

(4)  Often  appear  to  have  the  desired  effect. 

/.   Wallace's  objections    (See  Tropical  Nature). 

(i)    Theory  can   only  apply  to  the   more  intelli- 
gent animals. 

(2)  Brilliancy  of  males  due  to  lack  of  need   of 

protection,  absence  of  selection. 

(3)  Brilliancy  of  males  correlated   with  greater 

vigor,  therefore  preserved  by  natural  se- 
lection. 

(4)  No   evidence  of  females   being  affected   by 

display. 

(5)  Display  merely  due  to  general  excitement. 

(6)  Sexual   selection   nullified   by  natural  selec- 

tion. 

(7)  Every  bird  finds  a  mate  sooner  or  later. 

(8)  Impossibility  of    uniformity  of  taste   in   all 

females  of  a  species. 

g.  Romanes's  reply. 

(i)    Pattern  of  colors  cannot  be   due   to   vigor, 
e.  g.  Peacock,  Angus  phaesant. 

(2)  Remarkable  elaboration  of  structures,  e.  g, 

the  Bell-bird. 

(3)  Objection  7  begs  the  question. 

(4)  Decorative  (as  distinguished  from  brilliant) 

coloring,  melodious  song  (as  distinguished 
from  cries),  arborescent  antlers  (as  dis- 
guished  from  merely  offensive  weapons), 
and  the  Hke,  cannot  be  explained  by  nat- 
ural selection. 


46 

vi.    Isolatio7i  or  Segregatioji. 

(Lloyd  Morgan,  Animal  Life  and  Intelligence,  pp.  99-112; 
Romanes,  Physiological  Selection,  Journ.  Linn.  Soc,  Zool., 
Vol.  19,  pp.  337-411,  1886;  GULICK,  Divergent  Evolution 
through  Cumulative  Segregation,  Journ.  Linn.  Soc,  Zool., 
Vol.  20,  pp.  189-274.) 

The  difficulties   of  Natural   Selection  viewed  as  the 
sole  cause  of  evolution. 

(i)  The  difference  between  natural  species  and 
domesticated  varieties  in  respect  of  fer- 
tility when  crossed. 

(2)  General  inutility  of  specific  characters. 

(3)  Swamping  effects  of  intercrossing. 

Importance  of  Segregation,  or  the   prevention  of  in- 
tercrossing, in  the  origin  of  domesticated  varieties. 

Modes  of  Segregation  in  nature. 

Geographical. 

Variations  of  habits. 

Preferential  mating. 

Particulate  inheritance. 

Physiological  isolation. 
Evidences  for  physiological  isolation. 

Immense  number  of  variations. 

Sexual  organs  most  variable. 

Variation  often  toward  sterility. 


47 
vii.    Inheritance  of  Acquired  Chaj'acters. 

(Lamarck,  Philosophic  Zoologique;  See  also  Translation  in 
American  Naturalist,  Vol.  22,  pp.  960-972  and  1054-1066; 
Darwin,  The  Origin  of  Species;  Darwin,  Animals  and 
Plants  under  Domestication;  Eimer,  Organic  Evolution; 
Cope,  Origin  of  the  Fittest ;  Osborn,  Palaeontological  Evi- 
dence for  the  Transmission  of  Acquired  Characters,  Amer. 
Nat.,  Vol.  23,  pp.  561-566;  Ball,  Are  the  Effects  of  Use  and 
Disuse  Inherited.'  Osborn,  Are  Acquired  Variations  Inher- 
ited.' Amer.  Nat.,  Vol.  25,  pp.  191-216;  Spencer,  The  Prin- 
ciples of  Biology,  Vol.  i,  pp.  402-475;  Spencer,  Factors  of 
Organic  Evolution;  Weismann,  Essays  upon  Heredity,  Vol. 
I,  pp.  387-448;  Poulton,  Theories  of  Evolution,  Proc.  Bos- 
ton Society  of  Natural  History,  Vol.  26,  pp.  371-393 ;  Spen- 
cer, The  Inadequacy  of  Natural  Selection,  Contemporary 
Review,  Vol.  63,  pp.  153-166  and  439-456;  Romanes,  Mr. 
Herbert  Spencer  on  "  Natural  Selection,"  I.e.,  pp.  499-517; 
Spencer,  Professor  Weismann's  Theories,  /.  c,  pp.  743-760; 
Romanes  and  Hartog,  The  Spencer-Weismann  Controversy, 
I.e.  Vol.  64,  pp.  50-59;  Weismann,  The  All-Sufficiency  of 
Natural  Selection,  /.  e.,  pp.  309-338  and  596-610 ;  Spencer, 
A  Rejoinder  to  Professor  Weismann,  /.  u,  pp.  893-912  ;  Ro- 
manes, Weismannism  ;  Romanes,  Darwin  and  after  Darwin, 
Vol.  2  ;  Hyatt,  Phylogeny  of  an  Acquired  Characteristic, 
Proc.  Amer.  Phil.  Soc,  Vol.  32,  pp.  349-647,  1894  ;  Cope,  Pri- 
mary Factors  of  Organic  Evolution;  Bailey,  Plant-Breed- 
ing-) 

a.    Evidence  in   favor  of  the  inheritance  of  acquired 
characters. 

(i)    Indirect  evidence. 

"Appearances." 

Apparent    uselessness    of    nascent    adapta- 
tions (palaeontology). 

Reflex  actions. 

Instinct. 

(2)  Direct  evidence. 

Inherited  effects  of  use  and  disuse. 

Climate. 

Food. 

(3)  Experimental  evidence. 
Brown-Sequakd's  experiments. 


48 

Repetition  of  these  by  Romanes. 
Experiments    on  plants,    Hoffmann,    Car- 

RIERE,    BUCKMAN. 

b.  Evidence  against  the  inheritance  of  acquired  char- 
acters. 

(i)    Indirect  evidence. 

Theoretical  difficulties  in  the  way  of  inher- 
itance of  acquired  characters. 

(2)  Direct  evidence. 

Migration  of  germ-cells. 

Early  differentiation  of  germ-cells. 

(3)  Experimental  evidence. 

Negative  results  obtained  by  Romanes. 

Graft-hybridization. 

Transplantation  of  ovaries. 

Transfusion  of  blood. 

Transplantation  of  ova,  Heape  and  Buck- 
ley. 

Amputations,  Weismann. 

c.  Summary. 

Small  amount  of  evidence  in  affirmative. 

Inheritance  of  acquired  characters  involves  a  theory 
of  pangenesis. 

viii.    Cofistttntional  Tendency. 

(MiVART,  Genesis  of  Species  ;  Hyatt,  Genesis  of  the  Arietidae, 
Smithsonian  Contriljutions,  673,  1889;  Hyatt,  Bioplastology 
and  the  Related  branches  of  liiologic  Research,  I'roc.  l^oston 
Society  Natural  History,  Vol.  26,  pp.  59-124  ;  Weismann,  Ger- 
minal Selection.) 

Innate  tendency,  Mivart. 
Perfecting  principle,  Nacem. 


49 

Youth,  maturity,  and  senescence  of  species,  Hyatt. 

Germinal  selection,  Weismann. 
General  Conclusions. 

The  prime  factors  in  organic  evolution  are  Variation 
and  the  Struggle  for  Existence  with  the  resulting 
Natural  Selection. 

Other  important  factors  are  Sexual  Selection,  Segre- 
gation, Panmixia,  and  the  Reversal  of  Selection. 

Supposed  effects  of  the  inheritance  of  acquired  char- 
acters and  of  constitutional  tendencies  improbable. 

The  doctrine  of  evolution  well  founded  in  fact  and  es- 
tablished in  theory. 

IX.  DEDUCTIONS  FROM  THE  THEORY  OF 

EVOLUTION. 

Ontogeny  and  phylogeny. 

Significance  of  Sex. 

Origin  of  death. 

Color. 

Relations  of  animals  and  plants. 

Social  evolution. 

Philosopical  results. 


"  There  are  more  things  in  heaven  and  earthy  Horatio, 
Than  are  dreamt  of  in  your  philosophy.^'' 


/ 


SYLLABUS    OF   LECTURES   IN    THEORET- 
ICAL BIOLOGY. 


SUPPLEMENT,   J  897. 


The  following  section  may  he  substituted  for  the  corresponding  one  in   the 
text,  pages  17  to  27. 

iv.      Theories  of  Heredity  and  Development. 

(For  list  of  references  see  page  17.) 

1.  Importance  of  the  subject. 

2.  Requirements  of  a  theory  of  heredity. 

It  must  include  a  theory  of  variation. 

It  must  inckide  a  theory  of  development  in  the 
deepest  sense. 

It  must  explain  all  the  phases  of  development. 

It  involves  some  conception  of  the  essential  struc- 
ture of  living  matter. 

3.  The  fundamental  conceptions  in  theories  of  heredity. 

4.  Animism. 

Van  Helmont  (i  577-1644). 

5.  Physiological  units. 

BuFFON  (1 720-1 793),  organic  molecules. 

Spencer  (1864),  physiological  units. 

Haeckel  (1876),  plastidules. 

Weismann  (189 1,  1893),  biophors,  determinants. 

Nageli  {1884),  micellae,  micellar  threads. 

Darwin  (1868),  gemmules. 

De  Vries  (1889),  pangenes. 

BuTCHLi  (1892),  Andrews  (1897),  protoplasmic 
foam. 


Heredity  as  a  form  of  motion,  or  as  memory. 

Spexcer  (1864),  polarity  of  the  physiological  units. 
Congenital  variations  diK'  to  changes  in  polarity. 
Inheritance    of    acquired    characters    thus    ex- 
plained. 

Nageli  {1884),   morphogenic    stimuli    transmitted 
through  micellar  threads. 

Haeckel   (1876),  branched    wave  motion,  perigen- 
esis  of  the  plastidules.      Heredity  is  memory. 

Okr  (1893).      Heredity  is  habit. 

Cope. 

Objections  to  these  theories. 

a.  Their  fanciful  character. 

b.  All  founded  on  the  supposed  inheritance  of 
acquired  characters. 

c.  Absence  of  mechanism. 

Pangenesis. 

Democratus  (400  B.  C).     Seed  of  animals  formed 
by  contributions  from  all  parts  of  tlie  body. 

BuFFON  (1720-1793). 

Darwin  (1868).     Pangenesis  of  gemmules. 
Latent  gemmules. 
Acquired  characters  inherited. 

Galton  (1872).      Great  number  of  gemmules. 
Their  imaginary  character. 
Experiments  on  rabbits. 
Acquired  characters  seldom  inherited. 

Brooks  (1876,  1883).     Modified  pangenesis. 

New  gemmules  formed  only  under  unfavorable 
conditions. 

Hybrid  gemmules  induce  variation. 

Latent  gemmules  in  egg,  new  gemmules  in  sper- 
matozoon. 

Acquired  characters  not  inherited. 


De  Vries  (1889).     Pangenes  produced  by  nucleus. 
Summary. 

8.  Idioplasm  and  trophoplasm, 

Nageli    (1884).     Idioplasm   the  truly  living    sub- 
stance, trophoplasm  its  nutritive  matrix. 

Weismann    (1885).      Idioplasm    restricted    to  the 
nucleus,  and  distinguished  from  cytoplasm. 

Andrews   (1897).     Continuous   and  discontinuous 
substance. 

9.  Somatic  idioplasm  and  germplasm. 

Weismann   (1883,    1885,    1893).     Germplasm   dis- 
tinguished from  somatic  idioplasm. 

MiNOT  (1896).      Panplasm. 

10.    The  nucleus  as  the  seat  of  the  germplasm. 

O.  Hertwig  (1875).      Fusion  of  nuclei  during  fer- 
tilization of  the  egg. 

Van  Beneden  (1883).     Equal  number  of  chromo- 
somes in  male  and  female  pronuclei. 

Strasburger  (1884).     Same  process  in  plants. 

GuiGNARD  (1891).     Fertilization  in  the  lily. 
Later  observations  on  fertilization. 

BovERi  (1887),  Hertwig  (1890).     Maturation. 

Pfluger  (1883).     Isotropism  of  the  egg. 

Hertwig   (1884).      Position  of   the   spindle  deter- 
mines the  plane  of  cleavage. 

Brandt    (1877),    Nussbaum    (1884).      Enucleated 
fragments. 

Verworn,  Lillie.     Dependence  of  nucleus   upon 
cytoplasm. 


Haberlandt  (1877),  KoRSCHELDT  (1889),  position 
of  the  nucleus  in  relation  to  growth  and  nutri- 
tion of  the  cell. 

Hodge.     Ganglion  cells. 

BovERi  (1889).  Fertilization  of  enucleated  frag- 
ments. 

1 1.  Continuity  of  the  germplasm. 

Continuity  of  germ  cells. 

Contrast  between  pangenesis  and  continuity  of 
germplasm. 

Owen  (1849).      Continuity  of  germ  cells. 

Galton  (1872,  1876).      Stirp. 

Brooks  (1876,  1883).  Continuity  of  germ  cells 
and  their  contained  latent  gemmules. 

Jager  (1869).  Continuity  of  germ-protoplasm 
which  receives  flavor  and  odor  substances  from 
the  body  cells. 

Nussbaum  (1888).      Continuity  of  germ  cells. 

Weismann  (1883).      Continuity  of  germi:)lasm,  con- 
trasted with  continuity  of  germ  cells. 
The  germ  tract.     Diptera,  medusae,  ascaris. 
No  doubt  that  there  is  some  form  of   continuity. 

12.  Preformation. 

The    theory    of  continuity    leaves  unanswered  the 

questions  of  differentiation  and  variability. 
The  new  aspect  of  the  theory  of  preformation. 

Darwin  (1868).  Every  i')art  of  the  embryo  rep- 
resented in  the  egg,  by  one  or  more  gemmules. 

Brooks  (1883).      Similar  theory. 

His  (1875).     Theory  of  gemmules  rejected. 
Principle  of  the  differentiation  of   areas. 
Principle  of  iinccpial  growth. 


5 

Roux  (1888).     Experiments  with  frogs'  eggs. 
Distinction  between  quantitative  and  qualitative 

nuclear  division. 
The  mosaic  theory  of  development. 

De  Vries  (1889).      Intracellular  pangenesis. 

Differentiation   explained  by  migration   of  pan- 
genes. 

Weismann  (1893).      Inheritance   of  acquired   char- 
acters denied. 
Dynamical  and  pangenesis  theories  rejected. 
Variations  due  to  changes  in  the  germplasm. 
Amphimixis,  the  mingling  of  germplasms. 
Id,  the  unit  of  germplasm. 
The  id  composed  of  determinants. 
Biophor,  the  unit  of  protoplasm. 
Migration  of  biophors. 

The  process  of  ontogeny. 

Division  of  the  id. 
Division  of  determinants. 
Qualitative  and  quantitative  divisions. 
Reserve  germplasm. 

Effect  of  amphimixis. 

Struggle  of  the  ids  causing  variation. 
Homologous  determinants. 

Homodynamous. 

Heterodynamous. 
Variation  as   the  result  of  new  combinations 

of  old  characters. 

Reversion, 

Sexual  dimorphism. 

Regeneration. 

Budding. 

Alternation  of  generation. 

Preformation  of  every  detail. 


13-    Epigenesis. 

O.  Hertwig  (1892,  1894).      Doctrine  of  continuity 

accepted. 
Control  of  the  cell  by  the  nucleus. 
Units   of  idioplasm,  idioplasts  representing  cell 

characters  only. 
Theory  of  determinants  rejected. 
Comparison  of  organism  to  a  state. 

Evidence  concerning  the  mosaic  theory. 
For  the  theory  : — 

Experiments  of  Roux  on  frogs'  eggs.     Forma- 
tion of  half  embryos. 

Crampton  on  Ilyanassa. 

E.  B.  Wilson  on  normal  cleavage  of  Nereis. 

Presence  of  independently  variable  parts. 

Against  the  theory  : — 

a.  In  normal  development. 

Lack  of  correspondence  between  first  cleav- 
age plane  and  any  plane  of  adult  bod)-. 
Miss  Clapp  on  toad  fish. 
Morgan  on  frogs  and  tclcosts. 

• 

Variations  in  cleavage. 

H.  V.  Wilson  on  sea  bass. 
Jordan  on  Amphibia. 
E.  B.  Wilson  on  Amphioxus. 
Three  types  with  all  grades  between. 
All  result  in  normal  embryos. 

b.  Experimental  evidence. 
Effects  of  pressure. 

Driescii,  on  cchinoderm  eggs, 
Hertwig  on  frogs'  eggs. 
E.  B.  Wilson  on  Nereis. 
Morgan  on  Fundulus. 


Isolation  of  blastomeres. 
ZojA,  on  medusae. 
Wilson,  on  Amphioxus. 
Morgan,  on  Fundulus. 
Driesch,  on  Echinus. 
Driesch  and  Morgan,  on  Beroe. 
LoEB,  on  sea-urchins. 

c.    Summary  of  the  evidence. 

Result  unfavorable  to  the  theory  of  qualita- 
tive divisions. 
External  conditions  a  factor  in  differentiation. 

Hertvvig's  theory  of  development. 

Effects   due   to  the  constitution   of   the  egg, 

yoke,  shape,  etc. 
New  relations  established  by  cleavage. 
"  The  differentiation  of  the  cell  is  a  function 

of  its  position."     (Driesch). 
No  preformation  of  the  embryo  in  the  ^g^. 
Each    stage    in   development    determines    the 

next  stage. 
Development  purely  a  process  of  epigenesis. 

Driesch  (1894).     All  nuclei  equivalent. 

Reactions  of  the  idioplasm  and  the  cytoplasm 
upon  one  another. 

Pre-existing  differences  of  the  cytoplasm  con- 
dition the  activity  of  the  idioplasm  in  the 
different  regions  of  the  egg. 

Wilson  (1896). 

Relations  of  blastomeres  not  purely  mechan- 
ical. 

Differentiation  of  cytoplasm  in  one  stage  has 
a  determining  influence  upon  the  next. 

Nucleus  undergoes  a  change  during  develop- 
ment, but  not  because  of  qualitative  divi- 
sions. 


14.    General  summary. 

Two  factors  in  development  —  the  nature  of  the 
idioplasm,  and  the  stimuli  affecting  it. 

Similarity  of  parent  and  offspring  due  to  common 
origin  of  germinal  idioplasm,  and  to  similar  con- 
ditions of  development. 

Changes  in  conditions  affecting  germplasm  may 
induce  inlieritable  variations. 

Balance  of  evidence  favors  epegenesis. 

Much  still  to  be  learned. 

ERRATA. 

Page  5  add,  —  Andrews,  The  Living  Substance.     Supple- 
ment to  Journal  of  Morphology,  Vol.  12,  No.  2, 
1897. 
Pages  6,  10,  II.      For  Bovari,  ;r«^  Boveri. 
Page  12.     For  Young  nv?c/ Yung. 
Page  12  add,  —  Semper,  Animal  Life. 
PouLTON,  Colour  in  Animals. 
Sachs,  Lectures  on  the  Physiology  of  Plants.     1S87. 

Lecture  39. 
Davenport,  Experimental  Morphology.      1897. 
T.  H.   Morgan,  Development   of  the   Frog's    Egg. 

1897- 
Page  15.      For  Acclimatisation  /yv?^/ Acclimatization. 
Page  28  add,  —  Geikie,  The  Founders  of  Geology.     1897. 
Page  29  add,  —  Bi:di)ari),    Te.xt-book   of    Zoogeography. 

1895. 
^^'^'i<^  35  add,  —  Bailev,  Survival  of  the  Unlike. 
Page  46  add,  —  Romanes,  Darwin  and  after  Darwin. 

Vol.  3,  1897. 
Page  47  add,  —  Lloyd  Morgan,  Habit  and   Instinct  ;  Os- 

IJORN,  Organic  Selection.     American  Naturalist, 

November,  1897. 


/