Skip to main content

Full text of "The Thematic Apperception Test The Theory And Technique Of Interpretation"

See other formats


THE THEMATIC 
APPERCEPTION TEST 



THE THEMATIC 
APPERCEPTION TEST 

The Theory and Technique 
of Interpretation 

By 
SILVAN S. TOMKINS, Pn.D. 

Research Associate, College "Entrance 

Examination Board; 

Visiting Lecturer in Clinical 

Psychology, Princeton University 

Formerly Lecturer in Clinical Psychology 

Harvard Psychological Clinic 

Harvard University 

With the Collaboration of 

ELIZABETH J. TOMKINS, B.A. 




GRUME & STRATTON 
JMew York and London 



Library of Congress Catalog Card No. Med47-3536 



Copyright 1947 
GRUME & STRATTON, INC. 

381 Park Avenue South 
New York 16, New York 

First printing, October 1947 
Second printing, September 1948 

Third printing, May 195 O 

Fourth printing, November 1952 

Fifth printing, October 1955 

Sixth printing, October 1959 

Seventh printing, May 1962 

Eighth printing, July 1965 



Printed in U.S.A. (E-W) 



To 
ROBERT W. WHITE 



PREFACE 

This is a book about a test which is barely fifteen years old a promising 
adolescent emerging from the rites of puberty. The TAT has not yet attained 
full stature, that is certain, and perhaps this book should have awaited its 
further development. It is with the hope that the maturation of the test 
might be accelerated that I venture to publish a series of lectures delivered to 
a seminar on the diagnosis of personality. Many of the characteristics of 
lectures will be found in these pages more questions are asked than answered, 
hypotheses are illustrated more than proven, much that might have been 
mentioned briefly is laboriously illustrated, and what might have been elabo- 
rated is expressed obliquely. It is a workbook and not a compilation of 
established doctrine. I have addressed myself to two audiences: those who 
are completely unfamiliar with the TAT and research psychologists whose 
interest I hoped to enlist in the exploration of a host of problems problems of 
test interpretation which are also of import for the science of which the test 
is a constituent element. Because of these aims, the psychologist who is long 
familiar with the technique of interpretation and whose interests are primarily 
diagnostic may find too much that is well known and too much that is remote 
from his immediate clinical concern. 

There is much that should have been said which has not been said. 
Evidence is sometimes too slender to support generalization. This is due in 
part to my ignorance, in part to our collective ignorance. In some measure 
this was necessitated by the exigencies of publication. Complete documenta- 
tion would have necessitated an encyclopedia rather than a book. For this 
reason the reader is asked to take much on faith. It is proper therefore that 
he be told something of the larger sample on which this work is based. I have 
analyzed over 300 protocols of individuals of both sexes who ranged in age 
from 5 to 55 years. Approximately 200 were normal and about 100 were 
psychoneurotic or presented behavior problems. There are no more than ten 
psychotic records in this sample; this is the area in which my experience is 
most seriously limited. More important than the\ number of records is the 
care with which one has examined the history of the individual who told 
the stories. I have frequently compared my interpretations with material 
gathered by other clinicians who employed a variety of techniques in the study 
of the same individual. When this was not possible I personally gathered 
other evidence by interview and clinical and experimental techniques. More 
specifically, these records were obtained from cooperative studies at the Harvard 
Psychological Clinic, from a series of records which I analyzed as consultant 
to the Dartmouth Eye Institute, from a group of young women at a junior 

vii 



viii PREFACE 

college, from industrial consultant work, vocational counseling, diagnostic 
testing of neurotics, and finally from the records collected by graduate students 
in the course of their clinical training. 

This is the work of one who turned from psychology to the study of phi- 
losophy some fifteen years ago when psychology seemed indifferent to the prob- 
lem of personality. The theory of value seemed to me an inquiry more pertinent 
to the study of man than did any investigation of either the higher or lower 
mental processes. It was in the course of postdoctoral work in philosophy that 
a reading of Explorations in Personality by Henry A. Murray and the workers 
of the Harvard Psychological Clinic renewed my enthusiasm for the science 
I had found so unrewarding as a graduate student. Under the guidance of 
Robert W. White I resumed my study of psychology and learned from him 
the technique of interpretation of the TAT. His friendship sustained the 
effort of a decade which is reflected in these pages the effort of a philosopher 
to acquire the competence of a professional psychologist. His careful and 
unhurried work provided at once a model and support for one who was 
temperamentally unsuited to the prevailing climate of the academic scene the 
coercive clamor that the tempo of publication be ever accelerated. For these 
reasons I have dedicated this book to Robert W. White, Director of the 
Harvard Psychological Clinic. 

My greatest single indebtedness is to my wife, Elizabeth Tomkins, who 
collaborated with me. Chapters VI, VII, VIII, and IX are as much her work 
as mine. She analyzed mountains of protocols, batded against my obscurities 
and infelicities of expression and rewrote large sections of the manuscript. 
These labors are gratefully acknowledged by her name on the tide page. 

My debt to Henry A. Murray is great. It was his work which turned me 
back to the study of psychology. In his seminars I have learned much of what 
I know of personality. His TAT workshops provided the stimulation for 
many of the ideas in this work which are original. Though much of what 
I have said diverges in varying degrees from his doctrine, the intellectual 
ferment created by his genius provided the conditions necessary for creative 
effort. His work with the TAT opened vistas of the possibilities inherent in 
protective techniques which none of the workers of the Harvard Psychological 
Clinic will forget. 

Any book is necessarily a social undertaking into which one's colleagues 
are inevitably drawn. My debts are many. To my friend Sheldon J. Korchin 
I owe thanks for heroic efforts in coping with my obscurantism, which he 
reminded me is a privilege reserved for philosophers. Chapter V owes any 
clarity that it may possess to the careful criticism of Eugenia Hanfmann, 
Robert W. White, and Eli Robins. Chapter I is better than it might have been 
for the incisive critique of Leo Postman. I called upon the experience of 



PREFACE iX 

Morris Stein in connection with Chapters VIII and X. To Thelma G. Alper 
and her husband, Abraham Alper, I acknowledge an indebtedness for their 
extremely generous expenditure of time and energy when my own energy was 
at its lowest ebb. I am grateful to Mrs. Korchin for her assistance in reading 
proof. Mrs. Hahn prepared the index for which I want to thank her again 
since this is the second time she has volunteered her good services. To Mrs. 
Brecher I owe the typing of this manuscript, and to her husband, Melvin 
Brecher, I am grateful for countless assists in the birth process. To Henry 
Stratton I should like to express my gratitude for his infinite tolerance in 
bearing with delays in the preparation of this manuscript which tried his 
patience and mine. 

S. S. T. 

April 1947 

Cambridge, Massachusetts 



CONTENTS 

PAGE 

PREFACE vii 

CHAPTER 

I. History and Development of the Thematic Apperception Test. . i 

II. The Technique of Administration 21 

III. The Scoring Scheme 26 

IV. Introduction to the Technique of Interpretation 42 

V. Level Analysis 55 

VI. Diagnosis of Personality: The Region of the Family 109 

VII. Diagnosis of Personality: The Region of Love, Sex, and Marital 

Relationships 153 

VIII. Diagnosis of Personality: The Region of Social Relationships. . . . 201 

IX. Diagnosis of Personality: Work and Vocational Setting 234 

X. Diagnosis and Psychotherapy 266 

REFERENCES 287 

INDEX 293 



CHAPTER I 

HISTORY AND DEVELOPMENT OF THE THEMATIC 
APPERCEPTION TEST 

A. PRECURSORS OF THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST * 

Sensation and perception, long the concern of philosophy, claimed the 
exclusive energies of the infant science, experimental psychology, for the first 
two decades of its neonatal existence. In the closing decades of the nineteenth 
century, however, more complex and higher mental processes were brought 
under experimental scrutiny. Memory, attention, reaction, and feelings were 
yielding to experimental control. 

It was in 1879 that the first free-association experiment was performed. 
Francis Galton (33) was the experimenter; his subject, himself. Prophetic 
were his findings that associations which recurred several times, over a four 
month period, could be traced largely to his boyhood and youth, and associa- 
tions which occurred only once stemmed from more recent experience. The 
diagnostic power of this method did not escape Galton. "It would be very 
instructive to print the actual records at length, made by many experimenters 
. . . but it would be too absurd to print one's own singly. They lay bare the 
foundations of a man's thoughts with more vividness and truth than he would 
probably care to publish to the world." But he pursued this no further. The 
study of individual differences in imagery proved more congenial to Galton. 

By the turn of the century, experimental psychology was invading the 
"highest" mental process. The issue was an experimental psychology of 
thought. This was for some time experimental psychology's highest reach. 

It was the educator who first turned the psychologist's attention to more 
complex problems. Binet's scale for measuring general intelligence was in part 
an outgrowth of his work as a member of a commission appointed in 1904 
by the French Minister of Public Instruction. It was the commission's assign- 
ment to make recommendations for the instruction of feeble-minded children 
in the public schools of Paris. A year later, in July 1905, the school authorities 
of Breslau asked Ebbinghaus (26), among others, to investigate the fatigue 
effects of the continuous five-hour session in the schools of that city. In the 
course of this investigation Ebbinghaus developed what he called "a real test 
of intelligence ... a simple, easily applied device for testing those intellectual 
abilities that are fundamentally important and significant both in the school 
and in life." 

* This survey of the history of the Thematic Apperception Test systematically excludes 
precursors of other protective techniques. Several reviews of the history of the Rorschach 
and other protective techniques have appeared elsewhere. 

I 



2 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

The stimuli requisite for the study of personality came in large part from 
the mental hospital. The mental testing movement was too narrowly con- 
ceived. Its scope was defined by educators and psychologists alike, primarily 
in terms of intellectual function. As a consequence the study of personality 
fell to the psychiatrist, expert primarily in the biological sciences. His was 
the responsibility for applying a nonexistent science of personality. The psy- 
chologist was both unwilling and unable to provide either a theoretical or 
experimental foundation for psychopathology. 

In such a climate of psychological opinion it was inevitable that techniques 
and findings full of promise for the study of personality would remain unno- 
ticed, to be rediscovered when the science was ready. This was the case with 
Britain's (17) paper, "A Study of Imagination," which appeared in 1907 in 
The Pedagogical Seminary. He presented a series of nine pictures to a group of 
boys and girls ranging in age from 13 to 20 years and asked them to write 
stories suggested by the pictures presented. The stories were analyzed accord- 
ing to their use of names of persons or animals, their use of the first person, 
use of details of the pictures, their imaginative quality, their unity, length, 
explanatory power, and their use of religious, moral, and social elements. 
Significant sex differences were found. Girls' stories revealed more religious, 
moral, and social elements, more interest in clothes and the preparation of 
food; -boys were more interested in the consumption of food. The stories told 
by the girls were full of pity, sadness, and the fear of being left alone. Brittain 
considered this "almost a neurotic tendency" caused by a society which blocked 
normal social and physical activity for girls. He concluded that the correspond- 
ence of a particular kind of physical activity, affective life, and imaginal activity 
suggested some causal relationship. He envisioned the possibility that these 
three aspects of life were mutually interactive. 

One year later there appeared a similar study by Libby (52), who investi- 
gated the relation between the imagination and the feelings of school children. 
He found that the stories of younger children were "objective," those of the 
older children more "subjective," and that the significant difference occurred 
between the thirteenth and fourteenth year. Libby's interests in this study 
were developmental. He thought he had proved that the imagination of 
pupils in high school was not poorer than that of grade school pupils. 

A variant of the story technique was reported by Clark (19) at the ninth 
International Congress of Psychoanalysts in 1925. He used what he called 
the "phantasy method" for the psychoanalysis of narcissistic patients who were 
incapable of developing a transference neurosis. He requested his patients 
to imagine themselves as infants and to report the feelings and attitudes which 
very young infants might have. He found that by this indirect method he was 
able to evoke material permeated with the emotional feelings of infantile life 



HISTORY AND DEVELOPMENT 3 

which enabled him to cure patients otherwise inaccessible to psychoanalytic 
therapy. 

Brittain's original technique remained unexploited for twenty-five years. 
It was rediscovered by the psychiatrist Schwartz (92), working at the Clinic 
for Juvenile Research in Detroit. In the overcrowded conditions of the 
juvenile court clinic he did not have sufficient time to develop the rapport 
requisite for psychiatric interviewing, As an aid in initiating the interview 
of delinquent children he developed the Social Situation Picture Test. This 
consisted of eight pictures representing situations most frequently encountered 
in the histories of delinquents. The subject was first asked to describe what 
he saw, then to tell what the boy in the picture was thinking. On the basis 
of the subject's responses, the examiner then questioned the subject further, 
following up every "lead" suggested by the child's answers. The child was 
then asked to tell what he would think and do if he were the boy in the 
picture. The test was never widely used by psychologists, perhaps because 
of the limited scope of the pictures but more probably because standardization 
was made difficult by the serial questioning in which the nature of each 
successive question depended on the child's response to the previous question. 

B. THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

The first publication on the Thematic Apperception Test by Morgan and 
Murray (62) appeared in 1935. The test, they said, was based on the well 
known fact that an individual confronted with an ambiguous social situation 
and required to interpret it was likely to reveal his own personality in this 
process. While interpreting the objective situation the individual was apt 
to be less defensive, less aware of the scrutiny of the examiner, and conse- 
quently more likely to reveal much of his own inner life. At first, subjects 
were instructed to interpret the action in each picture and to make a plausible 
guess as to the preceding events and the final outcome. Only through experi- 
ence was it learned that much more was revealed if the subject was asked 
to create a dramatic story. 

Three years later, in Explorations in Personality (63), the preliminary 
Thematic Apperception Test * results were integrated with the general theory 
of personality developed by Murray and the workers at the Harvard Psycho- 
logical Clinic. 

Since 1938 the scope of inquiry has expanded, the pace of research quick- 
ened. The TAT was employed in the study of a wide variety of psycho- 
pathological syndromes: hysteria, anxiety hysteria, and obsessive-compulsive 
neurosis (2), schizophrenic psychosis (6, 12, 37, 71, 78), head injury (73), 
psychopathic delinquency (49), stuttering (75), and mental deficiency (86, 88, 

* Hereinafter referred to a$ TAT. 



4 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

89). The TAT has proven a useful instrument in the exploration of such 
diverse areas as child development (83), social attitudes and sentiments (14), 
assessment of military personnel (39, 67) and, most recently, culture and 
personality (40, 68). 

C. PROBLEMS OF RELIABILITY 

There is a twofold problem in connection with the reliability of the TAT. 
First, to what extent do independent analysts agree with each other in their 
interpretations of the same TAT protocols? Second, what is the degree of 
similarity between TAT protocols of a given subject in successive administra- 
tions of the test? Let us consider first the reliability of TAT interpretation. 

I. INTERPRETER RELIABILITY 

The extent to which independent interpreters of TAT protocols agree 
with each other will depend somewhat on the nature of the protocol being 
interpreted, the training and competence of the interpreter, and the conceptual 
scheme employed in interpretation. As a consequence, the reliability coeffi- 
cients of correlation reported by various investigators are themselves variable, 
ranging from coefficients of +0.30 to +0.96. 

Sanford (84), employing the quantitative need-press analysis on the TAT's 
of children, reported a rank-order correlation of +0.57 for need ratings and 
+0.54 for press ratings by 4 judges rating the protocols of 10 subjects. May- 
man and Kutner (58) have reported high contingency coefficients for ratings 
of both formal and content characteristics of stories. (The coefficients of 
correlation were not reported in their preliminary note.) Mayman, in his 
review of Slutz's paper, reports the latter to have found good agreement 
between interpretations made independently by two examiners. Slutz (94) 
in her published abstract does not cite correlation coefficients. 

Combs (23) reported a reliability study on the ratings made by 4 judges 
on 10 protocols. The mean percentage of agreement between 3 of the judges 
and Combs was 60 per cent. Six months after his original analysis he reanal- 
yzed 100 TAT stories as a check on the reliability of his own ratings. The 
mean percentage of agreement with his own original ratings was 68.8 per cent. 
Both of these results seemed disappointingly low to Combs. 

Harrison and Rotter (39) independently analyzed the protocols of 70 sub- 
jects given five pictures in group administration. On the basis of these TAT 
stories, they rated candidates for Armored Officer Candidate School at Fort 
Knox on emotional maturity and stability. They employed a three-point 
scale and also a five-point scale. On the shorter scale, complete agreement 
was reached in 64 per cent of the ratings, partial agreement in 30 per cent and 
complete disagreement in 6 per cent. The contingency coefficient for the 



HISTORY AND DEVELOPMENT 5 

three-point scale, when corrected for broad categories by the method of Garrett, 
was +0.73. For the five-point scale, complete agreement was found in 43 per 
cent of the cases; complete disagreement, in no case. Considering all ratings, 
with agreement arbitrarily defined as a disparity of one point or less, and 
disagreement as a disparity greater than one, there was an essential concurrence 
in 74 per cent of the ratings and disagreement in 26 per cent. The corrected 
contingency coefficient for the five-point scale was +-77- 

Clark (21) reported tetrachoric correlations on the agreement of two sets 
of ratings on one story from 50 subjects. All tetrachoric correlations were 
-(-0.90 or above for the following categories: effect of the environment on 
the organism, reaction of the organism to the environment, adequacy of the 
principal character, and the nature of the endings. But on the rating of 
"needs" expressed in the story the correlation dropped to +0.30. Clark gives 
one example of the type of disagreement responsible for this drop in inter- 
interpreter reliability. In a story dealing with a man's wish to get married, 
Clark rated the wish as indicating two needs, affection and sensory gratifica- 
tion (sex), whereas her co-worker rated it simply as a need for affection. This 
is in part a matter of clarity of definition of the scoring categories. 1 

This type of unreliability typically disappears in cooperative research when 
co-workers have the opportunity of discussing the rationale of their ratings. 
Thus, in TAT workshops, it is common for the reliability of ratings to be 
very low at the beginning but to increase to respectable magnitude with 
practice. The exact degree of consensus is a function of the competence of 
the group and the time they spend in thrashing out their differences. At the 
Harvard Psychological Clinic some investigators have been able to achieve 
interinterpreter reliabilities as high as +0.95, But this hard-won group 
achievement will remain an essentially local phenomenon until TAT investi- 
gators agree on some mutually acceptable conceptual scheme for rating stories. 
At the moment each investigator is a law unto himself. Fruitful comparison 
of research findings is all but impossible without some degree of standard- 
ization. But it is not easy to assess the advantages of early standardization 
against the possible yield of the creative ferment of a longer period of free 
experimentation. 

The reliabilities reported above range from +0.30 to +-9& Since these 
reliabilities are a function of using diverse scoring schemes, of diverse case 
material, of judges of varying ability and acquaintance with the scoring 
schemes, it is not surprising to find that the reported rater reliabilities are so 
variable. Reliability in the rating of material as complex as TAT protocols 
will not come easily. It will be an achievement requiring the active coopera- 
tion of psychologists on a nation-wide basis. 



6 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

2. REPEAT RELIABILITY 

So much for interpreter reliability. Let us turn now to the problem of 
repeat reliability the degree of similarity between the TAT protocols of a 
given subject in successive administrations of the test. The degree of repeat 
reliability must necessarily depend on the basic stability of the personality 
and its fluctuations as a function of time. The more stable the personality 
the more we should expect repetitions of the test to yield similar protocols. 
If the personality is less stable we should expect repetitions of the test to reflect 
this fact in a lower degree of repeat reliability. In part the degree of repeat 
reliability also depends on the magnitude of the temporal interval between 
successive administrations of the test. If we were to administer the TAT to 
an individual at the age of 6 and again at the age of 30, we should certainly 
expect to find greater difference between the two protocols than if the test 
had been repeated one month after the first administration. Differences 
between successive administrations of the test may then be expected either if 
the individual is changing rapidly or if the change is slower but the test 
is repeated after a long period of time. Finally, we might well expect a 
decrease in repeat reliability if experimentally induced changes are introduced 
between successive administrations of the TAT. 

a) As a function of Time Between Successive Administrations 

In a study by the writer (99), 45 young women ranging in age from 18 
to 20 years were given the TAT by group administration. Three groups of 
15 members each were then chosen at random from the group of 45. The 
first group of 15 was given the test two months later; the second group, six 
months later; and the third group, ten months later. These stories were then 
rated according to Murray's quantitative need-press scheme. The ratings 
between the first administration and successive administrations were then 
intercorrelated. The reliability coefficients for the group given the test two 
months later was +0*80; for the second group, in which there had been a six 
month interval between repeated administration, the reliability coefficient fell 
to +0.60, and for the third group, after a ten month interval, to +0.50. The 
difference between repetition at two months and at ten months is clearly 
significant. Between two and six months and between six and ten months 
the differences are less reliable. It is clear, however, that as we increase the 
time between successive administrations the repeat reliability of the TAT 
declines. This however is a general group trend and need not be true of any 
particular individual. If the personality of the individual is extremely stable, 
the temporal interval between repeated administrations may make little or no 
difference. Let us examine the influence of this factor on repeat reliability. 



HISTORY AND DEVELOPMENT 7 

b) As a Function of the Stability of the Personality 

We shall compare the repeat reliability of three sets of data from indi- 
viduals whose personalities differed in their stability. First, a group of rapidly 
developing adolescents, second a neurotic whose personality underwent spon- 
taneous reconstruction, and third a neurotic of a particularly stable personality 
structure. 

1. Personality of low rigidity. Childhood and adolescence represent the 
period of maximal plasticity of the individual the organism is never again 
so malleable. We should therefore expect successive administrations of the 
TAT during this period to yield the lowest repeat reliability. This is in fact 
the case. Sanford (84) reported an average repeat reliability of +-4^ f & c 
needs expressed by children and adolescents who had been given the TAT at 
yearly intervals over a three year period. 

2. Personality of moderate rigidity. In the study of Joseph Kidd reported 
by White (104), the TAT was administered when Kidd was 19 and again 
when he was 22. This period was witness to a spontaneous process of recon- 
struction of his disorganized personality. Although psychoneurotic, his per- 
sonality was not particularly rigid. Consequently we might have anticipated 
marked changes when the TAT was repeated three years later. This was the 
case. In the earlier test there were three main themes: the longing for lost love, 
the transformation of cruelty and greed by the sympathetic interest of an 
older man, and a theme of sadism and masochism. In the second set of stories 
the first two themes had virtually disappeared. This probably reflected Kidd's 
working through the grief occasioned by an unhappy love affair and the 
improvement in his relation with his father, with less need for regeneration 
and a greater tolerance of himself. There was also more open expression of 
the aggressive and acquisitive wishes from which he had previously been 
rescued by the older man. This represents a modification of the remaining 
theme. White does not report the coefficient of reliability between these two 
administrations. Although there are continuities between these two sets of 
stories, it is evident that the repeat reliability is not high, though perhaps 
higher than the correlation of +0.46 reported for adolescents. It is clear that 
the changes in the personality after a three year period have been reflected 
in the second TAT and thereby reduce its repeat reliability. Let us consider 
now a personality of marked rigidity. 

3. Personality of marked rigidity. In a study by the writer (98), a psycho- 
neurotic subject, "Z," 1 8 years old, was presented five days a week with a 
different picture and asked to write a story about it. This was continued for 
a period of ten months. These were pictures other than those included in the 
TAT set and were used in order to assess the influence of the nature of 
the picture on the repeat reliability of the TAT. In addition the complete 



8 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

set (30) of TAT pictures was administered three times, at intervals o three 
months. The third administration was immediately followed by a fourth 
administration when the subject was under the influence of alcohol. More- 
over, the subject's dreams were recorded over the ten month period and the 
results of approximately seventy-five hours of additional testing and experi- 
mentation were available. In general, the main themes that appeared in the 
first 30 stories given at the beginning of the. investigation were repeated in 
the second, third, and fourth administrations of the TAT, as well as in the 
stories written daily. During a two week period of euphoria the subject was 
presented with very pleasandy toned pictures in an attempt to modify his 
typically unhappy stories, but despite this change of both pictures and mood 
the stories remained the same. The writer submitted half of all the stories 
to one rater and the other half to another rater. This procedure yielded a 
correlation coefficient of +0.91 between approximately 200 stories rated by one 
interpreter and 200 stories rated by another interpreter. It was clear that the 
constancy of these stories, despite differences in pictures, time of administration, 
mode of administration (spoken and written), condition of the subject (normal, 
euphoric, intoxicated, fatigued, etc.) was a function of the unusual rigidity 
of this particular person. This rigidity was the consequence of Z's neurosis, 
which we will discuss at greater length in another chapter. 

Thus we have seen that for rapidly developing adolescents the repeat 
reliability of the TAT is approximately +0.46. In the case of a psychoneurotic 
who underwent a spontaneous process of reconstruction during a three year 
period there was a relatively low repeat reliability, but when the personality 
is extremely stable the TAT will reflect such stability in a repeat reliability 
as high as a coefficient of correlation of +-9 I > m the case of stories told over 
a period of ten months. 

c) As a Function of Experimentally Induced Changes 

If the TAT were sensitive to transitory fluctuations in the feelings and 
moods of the individual we would expect these fluctuations to further reduce 
the repeat reliability of the test. This problem is most easily studied by intro- 
ducing experimentally induced changes between successive administrations 
of the TAT. 

Bellak (9) administered the TAT before and after sharp criticism of his 
subjects' stories. He found, following this insult, a statistically significant 
increase in the amount of aggression expressed in the stories. 

Rodnick and Klebanoff (76) administered a modified TAT to the best 
and most 'poorly adjusted members of an NYA camp, before and after experi- 
mentally induced frustration. The more poorly adjusted group showed, as 
a result of this frustration, a distinct decrease in themes of superiority, an 



HISTORY AND DEVELOPMENT 9 

increase of aggression, and a decrease of themes concerned with "emotional 
states." The better adjusted group showed an increase in the themes dealing 
with "emotional states" and no decrease in themes of superiority of the central 
characters in the stories. 

Thus we see that changes in the momentary situation of the individual 
being tested a second time may reduce the repeat reliability of the TAT. This 
is clearly the consequence of experimentally induced stress preceding the second 
administration of the TAT. 

We have examined the twofold problem of interpreter and repeat reli- 
ability. We have seen that the reported interinterpreter reliability coefficients 
ranged from +0.30 to +0.96. We assumed that this variability was a function 
of using diverse scoring schemes, diverse case material, and of interpreters of- 
varying ability and acquaintance with the scoring schemes. 

Repeat reliability was seen to depend in part on the time that elapsed 
between successive administrations, in part on the stability of the personality 
being tested, and also on the stability of the individual's environment. 

D. PROBLEMS OF VALIDITY 

Inasmuch as the TAT is designed to reveal the wishes, expectations, hopes, 
and fears which underlie overt behavior, the problem of validating the test 
results is peculiarly difficult. Such impalpable material as hopes and fears 
frequently cannot be direcdy observed in overt behavior and' have to be inferred 
from their effects on behavior. 

There are however, several avenues of approach open for the study of the 
validity of the TAT. First, we may compare the TAT with other material, 
such as past history or dreams, and determine the extent to which these find 
faithful reflection in the TAT protocol. Second, we may compare this tech- 
nique with the findings of other techniques administered to the same subjects. 
The TAT may be compared with the findings of other protective techniques, 
such as the Rorschach; it may be compared with psychoanalytic study of the 
same individuals; or it may be compared with the intensive case study which 
employs a wide variety of techniques converging on the same individual. 
Third, we may assess TAT results on groups or individuals whose charac- 
teristics are well known, and determine the extent to which these characteristics 
are faithfully reflected in the TAT. Fourth, validity may be assessed by the 
TAT's success in exploration and prediction on the frontier of scientific inquiry. 
Fifth, its validity must be considered iri the light of its interaction with other 
data and other techniques. More generally, we must consider the relationship 
of the TAT to the stage of development of the science of which it is a con- 
stituent part. Let us turn then to a discussion of these diverse aspects of the 
problem of validity. 



10 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

I. COMPARISON OF TAT WITH OTHER MATERIAL 

Inasmuch as fragments of the individual's past history, his present overt 
behavior, as well as covert fears and wishes, are all reflected in the mosaic of 
his TAT stories, we must compare the latter with material from other sources. 
Let us consider, first, past history material as it is revealed in written auto- 
biography and interview. 

a) Autobiographical and Interview Past History Material 

It must be admitted that any attempt to reconstruct the past history of the 
individual, whether it be on the basis of written autobiography or interview, 
is itself open to question. Whatever the validity of such an attempt, it is worth 
while to compare such data with the TAT. We may thereby emerge with 
a rough estimate of the degree to which the TAT reveals at least some of the 
individual's actual past history. 

Murray (66) has reported that the subject, in responding to the pictures, 
draws upon four major fields for his plots: books and movies, actual events 
occurring to friends or to family members, experiences in the subject's own 
life, and the subject's conscious and unconscious phantasies. 

Markmann (55) studied the relationship between three TAT pictures * 
and the past history of 40 individuals who told stories to these pictures. The 
past history had been determined through a combination of autobiography 
and interview. She discovered that the past history was more faithfully 
reflected in some pictures than others. Stories to picture i most frequently 
reflected the individual's actual past history. She further showed that the 
story was more likely to be a true portrait of the individual's past history, 
if certain themes were expressed. Thus all those who expressed aggression 
either toward their parents or toward the violin, in stories told to the first 
picture, were in fact aggressive in their childhood. But stories told to the 
same picture, which were concerned with a benefactor through whose help 
the child was enabled to succeed, were less valid. In i out of 3 cases this was 
found to have no basis in the past history of the individual who told such 
a story. It was not the case, however, that an aggressive story told to any 
picture meant that the individual had been aggressive. There is much work 
to be done following up these leads of Markmann's exploratory study. It is 
clear that certain past history data is reflected in the TAT. The conditions 
under which this happens need further exploration. 

Combs (23) has investigated the extent to which TAT stories were based 
on personal experience as this was recorded in autobiographies. He assumed 
that the autobiography represented a valid, if somewhat selected, description 

* Picture i, the boy and the violin; picture 6 BM, young man and elderly woman* 
picture 7 BM, young man and older man. 



HISTORY AND DEVELOPMENT II 

of the individual's experience. He found that approximately 30 per cent of 
the TAT stories contained significant material drawn from the subject's own 
life experiences as revealed by their autobiographies. This is said by Combs 
to be a conservative estimate, inasmuch as he was comparing "situations" and 
not attitudes or feelings in either TAT or autobiography. In terms of his 
criterion he found that picture 2, showing an adolescent with a book in her 
hand y encouraged the most transfer of personal experience. He attributed this 
to the fact that the girl seems to be a student, which seemed to Combs to 
confirm the finding of Symonds (95) that the best results are obtained with 
pictures containing characters with which the subject can most readily identify 
himself in terms of age, status, and general similarity. Combs found that 
picture n, the dragon in the chasm, gave the least frequent situational identifi- 
cation. Combs and Symonds are in all probability correct. But it must be 
remembered that if the TAT were too heavily weighted with pictures which 
elicited ready identification with the figures in the picture, much of the specific 
value of the test would be sacrificed. Thus Renaud (74) found responses to 
picture n of critical importance in differentiating head injury cases and 
psychoneurotics. This happened primarily because the scene was so remote 
from everyday life that it permitted the expression of impulses which were 
equally remote from the individual's overt behavior. The rationale of these 
bizarre pictures will be considered in a later chapter. 

It is clear, then, that at least 30 per cent of the TAT stories contain 
significant material from the individual's own life experience and that in the 
case of stories told to particular pictures, principally i and 2, this percentage 
may increase sharply. Further, within such stories, particular themes are more 
likely than others to represent past history material. 

b) Dreams 

If the TAT elaborates wishes which may never appear directly in overt 
behavior, we should expect some of these phantasies to be found in the indi- 
vidual's dreams. The dream, there is reason to believe, is the medium par 
excellence for the expression of wishes and fears ordinarily subject to some 
degree of inhibition in overt behavior. 

Sarason (87) compared the TAT stories with the dreams of the same 
individual. His subjects were 34 mental defectives. In general, dream material 
and stories were similar, even though all the major themes of the stories 
were not found in the dreams. In no case, however were the data from these 
two sources at complete variance. He felt that the similarity was sufficient 
to have demonstrated the validity of the test interpretations. 

In the intensive study of a single subject by the writer (98), which was 
mentioned previously, many of the major themes of the TAT appeared as 



12 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

manifest content of the subject's dreams but again, as in Sarason's study, not 
all the themes of the TAT appeared in the dreams. 

Thus we have found that sources as disparate as the past history and 
dream life of the individual find representation in the imaginative productions 
of the TAT. The TAT reflects but a fragment of his past history but more 
of his wishes than appear in his dreams. 

2. COMPARISON OF THE TAT WITH OTHER TECHNIQUES 

In attempting to validate the TAT, investigators have compared its find- 
ings with other projective techniques, with psychoanalytic studies and with 
intensive case studies of the individual employing a variety of techniques. 

a) Other Projective Techniques 

Harrison (38) has compared the TAT with Rorschach findings. He 
reports substantial overlap between the results obtained with the two tech- 
niques. Henry (40) also reported close agreement between TAT and Ror- 
schach findings on Navaho children. These results are in general in agreement 
with the writer's own experience. There is, however, a type of discrepancy, 
which I have noticed from time to time, that calls for more investigation. This 
is seen in the case of the individual who gives very few movement responses 
in his Rorschach record but who gives TAT stories suggestive of a very rich 
inner life. It is possible that for certain individuals one medium of expression 
is much preferred to the other, inasmuch as the writer has also seen cases 
in which there are many movement responses in the Rorschach record but 
very meager imaginative productions in the TAT protocol. 

b) Psychoanalysis 

Murray and Morgan (62) reported on a patient whose TAT stories 
showed all the chief trends which five months of psychoanalysis were able 
to reveal. To date this represents the only attempt at such validation. A 
systematic investigation by Bellak, who administered the TAT to individuals 
about to undergo psychoanalysis and planned to repeat the administration of 
the TAT after the termination of their psychoanalysis, was interrupted by 
World War II. This technique of validation is open to the criticism that one 
unvalidated method is being used to validate an equally unvalidated method. 
Much depends on the validity of psychoanalytic interpretation if we are to 
employ it as an instrument of validating the TAT. In addition, differences 
in interpretation among psychoanalysts further complicate the use of psycho- 
analytic findings as a technique of validation. 



HISTORY AND DEVELOPMENT 13 

c) Intensive Individual Case Study the Case of "Z" 

In the intensive case study of 2 by the writer, seventy-five hours of inter- 
views, tests and experimental procedures disclosed no material inconsistent 
with his TAT protocol. It has been the writer's experience that the intensive 
case study usually reveals much that is not found in the TAT and the latter 
contains material not revealed by the case study. There is of course much 
overlap. But more important than the overlap between TAT and other 
techniques is the interaction between them. The TAT usually throws new 
light on the other material and the latter may clarify the meaning of the TAT. 
Horn (42) demonstrated that such interaction exceeds the sum which both 
contribute to diagnosis when TAT and case material are used as independent 
techniques. This has also been the experience reported by Henry in his study 
of the Navaho and Hopi. 

3. COMPARISON OF TAT RESULTS WITH GROUPS OF KNOWN DIFFERENCES 

One of the most widely employed techniques of validation consists in 
administering an unvalidated technique to groups whose characteristics are 
already well known. If the results are in agreement with previous knowledge, 
evidence for the validity of the technique is considerably strengthened. This 
may be done in one of two ways. First, it may be done "blind" in an attempt 
at postdiction. The experimenter may analyze the TAT results without 
knowledge of the characteristics of the subjects and then compare his results 
with previously established findings in an attempt to discover similarities 
between the TAT and other data. The former method is the more convincing, 
although it suffers the disadvantages of error in interpretation of the TAT. 
There may in fact be evidence of similarity which the interpreter overlooked 
in "blind" analysis, but which might have been discovered had he known 
the characteristics of the tested group and been sensitized to these character- 
istics as he interpreted the TAT. Perhaps the best validation procedure 
would be the utilization of both methods first "blind" analysis and then a 
re-examination of the protocols in the light of further knowledge of the 
characteristics of the tested group. 

a) Postdiction of Groups of Known Differences 

Harrison, in conjunction with a qualitative study by Rotter (39), under- 
took a quantitative investigation of the validity of the TAT. The subjects 
were 40 patients at the Worcester State Hospital. Following his administra- 
tion and analysis of the test protocols, Harrison wrote a thumbnail personality 
sketch of each patient. These sketches included a description of characteristic 
traits, biographical facts, attitudes, level of intelligence, and personal problems 
and conflicts. The correctness of these items were then checked by an assistant 



14 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

against the hospital records. Eighty-two and five tenths per cent of the 
inferences made were correct. The correlation between guessed and actual 
I.Q.'s was +0.78. Inferences as to diagnostic classification were 75 per cent 
correct. There were no significant differences between the proportion of cor- 
rect inferences that he made concerning biographical items and personality 
or intellectual items. In another experiment by Harrison (37) an additional 
control was introduced. The test was administered by another person in order 
to eliminate the cues from face-to-face contact with the patients. He then did 
a completely blind analysis. With this procedure the percentage of correct 
inferences fell from 82 to 74 per cent, a statistically insignificant difference. 
But it would be surprising if face-to-face contact with the subject had not 
contributed somewhat to the write-up of the personality sketch. Although 
the difference is statistically insignificant in this experiment, if the experiment 
were replicated it is the writer's guess that this small difference would con- 
tinue to result from the employment of these two procedures. 

Murray and Stein (67) rated leadership ability in officer condidates in 
ROTC on the basis of the TAT. These ratings correlated +0.65 with inde- 
pendent judgements of these same men made by their officers. 

An impressive demonstration of the diagnostic power of the TAT was 
Henry's (40) blind analysis of the stories of Navaho and Hopi Indians. These 
reports were read by anthropologists who were familiar with the Navaho and 
Hopi cultures. In the opinion of these independent experts, the analysis based 
on TAT stories alone agreed essentially with all known facts available on 
the two cultures. In addition, Henry compared his interpretations with 
material already published on the Hopi and Navaho. The congruence of field 
anthropological studies and TAT findings is striking. As previously men- 
tioned, Rorschach findings on the Navaho were also compared with TAT 
results and disclosed substantial agreement. This comparison substantiated 
many of the psychological variables measured by the TAT which could not 
be validated by customary anthropological data. These findings revealed 
important psychological changes in children of Navaho society at different 
age levels at which cultural restraining forces exerted their influence. Henry 
concluded that the data were of sufficiently high validity to have justified the 
use of the TAT in the research on Indian education and recommended its 
further use in the study of culture and personality. 

Let us consider next those investigations in which the interpreter utilized 
the knowledge which was available on the group he was testing. 

b) Comparison with Groups of Known Differences 
Several investigators have analyzed the TAT with full knowledge of the 
known characteristics of the groups they have tested in an effort to determine 
the extent to which these characteristics were reflected in the TAT protocols. 



HISTORY AND DEVELOPMENT 15 

Richardson (75) found the TAT relatively useless for eliciting differences 
between stutterers and nonstutterers, though other tests did reveal significant 
differences between these two groups. 

Balken and Masserman (2) were able, by means of an analysis of the 
language of the protocols (the actual words used by the patients), to demon- 
strate statistically significant differences between patients diagnosed as con- 
version hysteria, anxiety hysteria, and obsessive-compulsive neurosis. They 
caution us, however, that their cases were relatively "pure" examples in which 
the dynamisms underlying these categories were particularly clear. For the 
more typical "mixed" cases it is less certain that the TAT or any other tech- 
nique could yield such differential diagnosis. 

Renaud (74) found significant differences between the TAT's of psycho- 
neurotics, brain disease cases, and head injury cases. The nature of some of 
these differences will be discussed in a later chapter. 

We see then that the TAT has to some extent been validated by both 
types of comparison with groups of known differences. Less seems to depend 
on the "blindness" of the analysis than on the nature of the groups studied. 
Thus Harrison's completely blind analysis of psychotics was correct in 74 per 
cent of the cases, whereas Richardson, with all knowledge available to him, 
was unable to differentiate between the TAT's of stutterers and nonstutterers. 
To what extent another investigator might have discovered differences we 
cannot say, inasmuch as the attempt has not been repeated. At our present 
stage of relative ignorance of TAT interpretation it is necessarily the case 
that these varying estimates of the test's validity are conservative. Further 
knowledge of the test will probably improve the validity of the inferences 
we can make from the TAT. 

A further problem in the use of this technique of validation is that which 
we raised in connection with the use of psychoanalytic findings. To what 
extent are the "known findings" themselves valid? The ratings of leadership 
by ROTC officers may or may not be valid. One of the lessons of the war 
experience was the uncertain validity of the judgements of army personnel 
which were used to validate psychological tests. Consider also the validity of 
hospital diagnosis on the basis of Kraeplinian categories. These admittedly 
leave something to be desired. Anthropological studies are also somewhat 
less than certain as a basis for validating other techniques, and, as we have 
seen, on certain aspects described by the TAT anthropological evidence was 
mute, so that the TAT had then to be compared with Rorschach findings. 
With Renaud's study we are on more solid ground; head injury, brain disease, 
and psychoneuroses are capable of relatively certain differentiation. Least 
controversial of the **known differences" is that between stutterers and non- 
stutterers; however, the test proved least valid in differentiating these groups. 
Inasmuch as other techniques did reveal differences between stutterers and 



l6 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

nonstutterers, it would be important to replicate this experiment in order to 
determine whether these results are peculiar to Richardson's sample, and 
further, if these two groups can not be differentiated by the TAT, to determine 
why this is so. Negative evidence has, in the history of science, not infre- 
quendy provided the opportunity for crucial investigation. 

4. THE TAT AS AN INSTRUMENT OF EXPLORATION AND PREDICTION 

Impressive as postdiction may be, the acid test of any technique is its 
usefulness hi exploration and successful prediction on the frontier of science. 
If and when, by use of the TAT, we are enabled to make significant contribu- 
tions to the science of which it is a constituent part, the test will have estab- 
lished its validity beyond question. 

To date such attempts are conspicuous by their absence. It is the con- 
viction of this writer that the TAT provides data of sufficient import and 
scope to justify a coordinated program of research on a nation-wide scale. The 
TAT is peculiarly sensitive in eliciting material which the individual ordinarily 
guards zealously from public scrutiny. Since this is the source of much of 
what we call mental disease it would be most rewarding if several thousand 
records of psychopathological syndromes of every variety were available for 
analysis. Such an enterprise would necessarily involve the energies of many 
individuals for many years. But the possible fruit of such labor might be a 
rectification of mental diseases which yielded truly homogenous entities. 
Once in possession of such knowledge the pace of significant research could be 
rapidly accelerated. Our position without such knowledge is not unlike that 
of a medical scientist who intended to study organic disease armed only with 
the knowledge that some diseases were characterized by subnormal tempera- 
tures, others by higher temperatures, and still others by unusually high tem- 
peratures, although most diseases seemed "mixed." This is perhaps a caricature 
of our position. But if evidence is needed to underline the urgency of this 
problem, let the reader spend an afternoon perusing the official dossier on any 
patient sent from one mental hospital to the next to come to rest finally in a 
state mental hospital. One typically finds a series of assorted diagnoses from 
each institution based on five to four decisions of an earnest but puzzled staff. 
Among these diagnoses will be many labeling the patient "mixed type" or 
with a more definite label qualified by many "trends" of other syndromes. 
This condition must of necessity continue until our classification of mental 
diseases allows less ambiguous diagnosis. 

5. INTERACTION OF THE TAT WITH OTHER DATA AND OTHER TECHNIQUES 

In the preceding sections we have examined the validity of the TAT 
as if the test were an isolated technique of investigation and to some extent 



HISTORY AND DEVELOPMENT Y] 

as if its validity were a static characteristic of the test itself. Both of these 
assumptions are to some extent untenable. Let us consider the latter assump- 
tion first. 

We cannot meaningfully inquire whether the TAT is a valid test, but 
we may ask whether inferences based on TAT stories are true. Similarly, 
we do not usually ask whether the experimental method is a valid method, 
but we do ask whether inferences based on the experimental method are likely 
to be true. There can be no guarantee that the inferences drawn from either 
the TAT or experimental data will necessarily be true. The history of science 
is witness to the frequency with which even the best experimental data have 
led to inferences which later proved to be false. It is even more certain that 
the data provided by the TAT have in the past, and will again in the future, 
prompt investigators to draw inferences which are untrue. But the value of 
a method, be it the experimental method or the procedure employed in the 
TAT, does not necessarily rise and fall with -each success or failure in 
interpretation. 

If the validity of our inferences is not guaranteed by the precision of our 
methods, on what does it depend? The meaning of any data stems ultimately 
from the fabric of interconnected propositions we call science and proximately 
from the hypothesis which we would relate to the already established system 
of accepted propositions. Any particular fact may become "crucial" only in 
the context of a hypothesis related to the larger matrix of previously established 
laws. Within such a context, the new fact both receives its significance and 
contributes to the further unification of its particular science. A collection of 
facts may all be "true," but such truth yields neither control nor prediction 
of any import. Facts provide the raw material, but not the mortar for the 
edifice of science. They are again crucial when the superstructure of this 
edifice must be tested. 

What consequences does this excursion into the philosophy of science 
entail for the problem of the validity of the TAT? Simply that the validity 
of inferences drawn from this test or any other test is limited by the stage of 
development of the science of psychology. No method, however precise, can 
lift a science by its bootstraps. Were we today in possession of a psychologic 
periodic table, the validity of inferences from the TAT or any other technique 
would be greatly increased by the general accruement of knowledge of per- 
sonality dynamics. But in the infancy of its development, the science is 
capable only of halting, uncertain, and f requently invalid inferences whatever 
the method employed. The experimental method, it can not be denied, is an 
impeccable method, but its application to personality study does not immedi- 
ately yield a full-blown science of personality free of invalid propositions. It is 
always much less difficult to devise techniques for answering questions than 



l8 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

to know what questions to put to Nature. In the TAT the writer has found 
answers to many questions, but only after he was able to ask the questions. 
We do not believe that the future of the TAT depends on the refinement of 
clinical intuition. Both TAT interpretation and clinical intuition itself can 
be no better than the general stage of development of the science permits. 

If validity is not a static characteristic of the TAT itself, we should not 
then treat the test as an isolated technique of investigation. Most of the studies 
cited in the preceding sections have examined the validity of the test as an 
independent instrument. "Blind" analysis may be impressive, but the value 
of a technique need not depend on its potency in splendid isolation from other 
techniques and other data. Let us consider one empirical study which presents 
us, in microcosm, with a paradigm of how increments in validity may accrue 
to inferences based on TAT stories. White (102) studied the extent to which 
hypnotic susceptibility might be correctly inferred from the TAT stories told 
to card 13 by hypnotic subjects. The TAT stories when first analyzed yielded 
a correlation of only +0.34 with hypnotic susceptibility. Predictions based on 
autobiographical and interview material were slightly better, the correlation 
being -f 0.40. When both sources of information were used the prediction 
improved to the extent of a correlation of 4--59 This increment was a 
function of combining different data and techniques. But White found he 
could improve the validity of inferences based on TAT stories alone through 
further study of the relationships between the stories and hypnotic susceptibility. 
He found that the 7 most hypnotizable subjects all stated explicitly in their 
stories that the hypnosis was a success, but the remaining 8 made this point 
incidental, merely implied it, expressed repugnance for hypnosis or explicitly 
declared it a failure. When this subsequently discovered relationship was 
used as a criterion, the correlation rose to +0.56, which was almost as high as 
the correlation of +0.59 when the original criterion plus autobiographical 
and interview material had been used as the basis of prediction. White then 
employed the new criterion plus autobiographical and interview material, and 
the prediction on this basis rose to +0.75. This is a striking demonstration, 
first, of the accrument of validity to inferences based on the TAT when this 
material is illuminated by other material, and second, of the accruement of 
validity to inferences based on the TAT alone after further study of the rela- 
tions between stories and the storyteller's hypnotic susceptibility. 

The latter suggests a more frontal attack on the problem of the validity 
of the TAT. Although we may be limited by the primitive state of our 
science, White's experiment shows us that we can improve the validity of our 
inferences by examining more carefully the relationships between the stories 
and the personalities of the subjects who tell them. We have barely scratched 
the surface of such inquiry. This reorientation would involve a strategic 



HISTORY AND DEVELOPMENT 19 

retreat. We would, for some time, have to relinquish our attempt to prove 
that the test was valid and turn our attention to the problem of why it was 
either valid or invalid. Frequently more can be learned from invalid test 
results than from valid inferences. In the technique of the questionnaire, 
for example, it is well known that subjects frequently rate themselves with 
a minimum of self insight. If we were to infer from these self ratings that 
the individual has presented us with a faithful portrait of his own personality, 
we might be mistaken as frequently as we were correct. But if we were to 
relinquish, temporarily, our aspiration to employ the questionnaire as a com- 
pletely adequate basis for valid inference and study the conditions under which 
the individual is likely to rate himself accurately and the conditions under 
which he will deceive himself or others, then ultimately we might make 
important contributions to the science of personality and at the same time be 
enabled to employ the questionnaire method with greater assurance. Similarly 
with the TAT, if more attention is given to its signal failures and the conditions 
under which inferences are grossly in error as well as the conditions under 
which inferences are valid, we may ultimately contribute not only to the 
greater usefulness of the TAT but, more important, to an understanding of 
the dynamics of personality. 

SUMMARY 

In our examination of the precursors of the TAT we saw that experi- 
mental psychology in the nineteenth century was more concerned with an 
examination of the lower and higher mental processes of the person than in 
the study pf the person himself. It was the educator who first turned the 
psychologist's attention to more complex problems. But the mental testing 
movement was so narrowly conceived that it fell to the psychiatrist, expert 
primarily in the biological sciences, to apply and develop a nonexistent science 
of personality. We also observed that in such a climate of psychological 
opinion, techniques and findings full of promise for the study of personality 
remained unnoticed, to be rediscovered some twenty-five years later when the 
science was ready. 

The first publication on the Thematic Apperception Test by Morgan and 
Murray appeared in 1935. Since that time the scope of inquiry has expanded, 
the pace of research quickened. The TAT has proven useful in diverse fields 
of inquiry. 

We examined the twofold problem of interpreter and repeat reliability. 
Reported interinterpreter reliability coefficients ranged from +0.30 to -{-0.96. 
We assumed that this variability was a function of using diverse case material, 
diverse scoring schemes, and of interpreters of varying ability and acquaintance 
with the scoring schemes. Repeat reliability was seen to depend in part on 
the time which elapsed between successive administrations, in part on the 



20 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

stability of the personality being tested and also on the stability of the indi- 
vidual's environment. 

We next examined the problem of validity. We found that sources as 
disparate as the past history and dream life of the individual found repre- 
sentation in the imaginative productions of the TAT. We saw further that 
comparison of the TAT with other techniques, such as the Rorschach, 
psychoanalytic, and the intensive case study, revealed considerable overlap and 
no evidence of inconsistency in findings. Comparing the TAT results on 
groups of known differences, we found evidence of substantial agreement, 
whether the analysis was done "blind" or with prior knowledge of character- 
istics of the tested groups. Only in the case of stutterers did the TAT fail 
to reveal differences from the TAT's of nonstutterers. It was suggested that 
this experiment be replicated. 

Attempts to use the TAT as an instrument of exploration and prediction 
were seen to be conspicuous by their absence. It was suggested that such 
exploration might profitably be directed toward the problem of the reclassifica- 
tion of mental diseases. 

Finally we examined the interaction of the TAT with other data and 
other techniques. We maintained that validity was a characteristic of infer- 
ences based on the TAT rather than a characteristic of the test itself. We 
maintained that the validity of such inferences was as dependent on the 
maturity of the science as on the adequacy of the method. We showed in 
an examination of an experiment that validity could be increased either by 
the accruement of data from other sources or from further study of the test 
itself. We suggested a strategic retreat involving a temporary abandonment 
of clinical application of the test in favor of a study of the conditions under 
which valid and invalid inferences might be drawn from the protocol. 



CHAPTER II 
THE TECHNIQUE OF ADMINISTRATION 

DESCRIPTION OF TEST MATERIAL 

The test consists o 29 pictures and one blank card. Cards i, 2, 4, 5, 
10, u, 14, 15, 16, 19, and 20, are used for both sexes and all ages. For cards 
3> 6, 7> 8, 9, 17, and 18, there are alternate forms marked "BM" for boys 
and older males, and "GF" for girls and older females. Card 12 has three 
forms marked "M" for males over 14, "F" for females over 14 and "BG" for 
boys and girls under 14. Card 13 also has three forms marked "MF" for 
males and females over 14, "B" for young boys and "G" for young girls. Thus 
there are four sets of twenty cards suitable either for men or women, boys or 
girls. The present series of pictures is the third revision of the first set 
distributed by the Harvard Psychological Clinic in 1936. Each set is divided 
into two series of ten pictures. The pictures of the second set were purposely 
chosen for their unusual and bizarre quality. 

ADMINISTRATION 

The subject, depending on his preference, may be seated in a chair or 
stretched out on a couch, either facing the experimenter or with his back to 
him. Murray (65) and Rotter (79) suggest that his back be to the experi- 
menter; Rapaport (71) prefers the person to sit up facing him. Rotter admits 
that some subjects dislike having the examiner behind them. In the experience 
of this writer, there are subjects who prefer the couch, others become anxious 
and refuse to lie down, and some reluctantly lie down but keep one foot on 
the floor to reassure themselves. Some individuals required to sit facing the 
examiner avoid his eyes and look into space, while subjects who prefer the 
face-to-face position will turn their heads around to achieve this if they are 
required to sit with their back to the examiner. 

INSTRUCTIONS 

The instructions, in one of the following forms, arc read slowly to the 
subject. Form A * (suitable for adolescents and for adults of average intelli- 
gence and sophistication): 

This is a test of imagination, one form of intelligence. I am going to show 
you some pictures, one at a time, and your task will be to make up as dramatic 
a story as you can for each. Tell what has led up to the event shown in the 

* Reprinted by permission of the Harvard University Press. 

21 



22 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

picture, describe what is happening at the moment, what the characters are 
feeling and thinking; and then give the outcome. Speak your thoughts as they 
come to your mind. Do you understand? Here is the first picture. 

Form B (suitable for children, for adults of little education or intelligence, 
and for psychotics): 

This is a storytelling test. I have some pictures here that I'm going to show 
you, and for each picture I want you to make up a story. Tell what has 
happened before and what is happening now. Say what the people are feeling 
and thinking and how it will come out. You can make up any kind of story 
you please. Do you understand? Well, then, here is the first picture. 

These are the instructions used by Murray, with the exception that any refer- 
ence to the time available to the subject has been omitted from these instruc- 
tions, since in our experience, brief stories cannot be lengthened, and stories 
which run over five minutes are sometimes too valuable to cut short. The 
exact words of these instructions may, as Murray suggests, be altered to suit 
the subject. In using the test with young children, the writer has achieved 
best results by turning the test into a game. If the intent is therapeutic and 
the child still avoids certain topics, it is possible to lure him into these regions 
by alternating the telling of stories between therapist and child. The therapist 
can then make up a story close to the child's problem and hesitate in telling it. 
The child, impatient either to get on with the game, or to outdo the therapist, 
will often volunteer to fill in the gaps. 

After finishing the first story, it is recommended by Murray that the sub- 
ject be commended and then reminded of the instructions; and that the 
examiner say very little for the rest of the hour except to inform him whether 
he is ahead or behind schedule, encourage him from time to time, and prompt 
him briefly if he has omitted some crucial detail. Rapaport, perhaps because 
he tests more difficult subjects, namely, psychotics, is more insistent that indi- 
viduals who refuse in one way or another comply with the instructions. The 
writer has not personally administered TAT's to psychotics, but, in his experi- 
ence with seriously disturbed neurotics, refusal *to comply can be changed 
neither by insistence nor entreaty. Resistance in the testing situation yields, 
if at all, only in a completely permissive atmosphere. Attempts by the 
examiner to elicit material which the neurotic is unable or unwilling to give 
usually result in more resistance and even greater impoverishment of material. 
Consider one aspect of compliance: the instructions ask that the subject speak 
his thoughts as they come into his mind. Lasaga (50), through the use of 
the inquiry, investigated the rejected ideas which first occurred to the subject 
on seeing a picture but which were not used in making up the stories. In 
some cases, the story proved to be a variant of the rejected idea or another 
aspect of the same idea. In other cases no light was shed on the patient's 



THE TECHNIQUE OF ADMINISTRATION 23 

personality by the rejected ideas. In general, however, Lasaga found that the 
rejected ideas taken together usually reflect the same preoccupations which 
the stories themselves revealed. 

Rapport with the subject is of critical importance. The writer has, on 
occasion, repeated the procedure with patients after a- better relationship has 
been achieved and obtained revealing protocols even though, the first attempt 
had been very disappointing. Murray recommends that the subject be praised 
for his efforts in order to stimulate his imagination. In general, we have found 
this to be advisable though individuals vary somewhat in the kind of approba- 
tion needed for their best effort. If some knowledge of the individual's basic 
needs is available before testing, this should govern the attitude which the 
examiner manifests toward the subject. Some subjects need respect; others 
sympathy and support, for their creative efforts. There are also subjects who 
respond best to a press of dominance from the examiner, but others, under 
such prodding, respond with negativism or complete withdrawal from the 
whole situation. If the subject is suffering acute anxiety or any other acute 
disturbance, testing is contraindicated since the stories will reflect only his 
specific preoccupation, giving so little elaboration that the dynamics of the 
state can not be inferred. In the case of a Marine, for example, who had 
returned from combat to find that his wife was in love with another man, 
every TAT story was nothing more than a description of one person who "was 
worried" and "wondered how everything was going to turn out." 

Ordinarily, at least one day should intervene between the first and second 
sessions, unless the stories are so short that the entire set of twenty may be 
finished within an hour. Generally, no testing session should exceed one hour. 
In certain cases, as many as three one hour sessions may be required to com- 
plete the series. The instructions for the second session are modified as follows: 
Form A: 

The procedure today is the same as before, only this time you can give freer 
rein to your imagination. Your first ten stories were excellent, but you 
confined yourself pretty much to the facts of everyday life. Now I would 
like to see what you can do when you disregard the commonplace realities and 
let your imagination have its way, as in a myth, fairy story, or allegory. Here 
is picture no. i 

Form B: 

Today I am going to show you some more pictures. It will be easier for you 
this time because the pictures I have here are much better, more interesting. 
You told me some fine stories the other day. Now I want to see whether you 
can make up a few more. Make them even more exciting than you did last 
time if you can like a dream or fairy tale. Here is the first picture. 

Card 1 6, the blank card, is accompanied by a special instruction. The examiner 
says, "See what you can see on this blank card. Imagine some picture there 



24 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

and describe it to me in detail." If the subject does not succeed in doing this, 
the examiner says, "Close your eyes and picture something." After the subject 
has given a full description of his imagery, the examiner says, "Now tell me 
a story about it." This modification of instructions is a type of "testing of 
limits." Many subjects will not comply; they may or may not be aware that 
they are not complying. But those who accept the instructions frequently tap 
remote recesses of their imagination which otherwise might not be reached. 

INQUIRY 

In this part of the procedure we suggest that the examiner follow up leads 
based on the hypotheses he has formulated in analyzing the stories. This may 
be done by direct question, free association, or by presenting the subject with 
special cards designed to evoke further phantasies on critical areas. Such a 
recommendation involves postponing the inquiry until the stories have been 
analyzed. Inquiry after each story, as suggested by Rapaport, will too fre- 
quently arouse suspicion and resistance. Many subjects interpret such frequent 
inquiry either as an indication that they are not satisfying the examiner, with 
a consequent decrease in productivity, or as an unwelcome limitation on their 
freedom. 

Murray recommends that inquiry be made into the sources of various 
stories. This is sometimes useful in identifying autobiographical material if 
it is not already available. Rapaport uses the inquiry, among other things, 
to discover whether lack of perceptual clarity represents pathological perceptual 
distortion. This would be most important in the case of psychotic patients 
and sometimes with psychoneurotics. In the inquiry, unclear verbalization 
as well as any lack of clarity in the meaning of the story should be investigated. 

RECORDING 

Recording of the stories and the inquiry is best done either by a stenog- 
rapher in another room, with transmission through a concealed microphone, 
or by wire, disk or tape recording devices. In our experience the presence of 
a microphone does not disturb most subjects, but there are some for whom it is 
better to use a concealed microphone. Writing down the exact words of the 
subject is all but impossible without proficiency in shorthand and the imaginal 
productivity of a facile subject may be seriously impaired if he has to be slowed 
down to the speed with which the examiner can write. If recording is imprac- 
ticable, we prefer that the subject write his own protocol in ink. In this way, 
many slips of the tongue which might otherwise be lost appear in the material; 
they can only be crossed out, not erased. Preliminary research by the writer 
indicates that, in general, there are no significant differences between stories 
written and spoken, although there are individual subjects who are more 
productive in one medium than another. 



THE TECHNIQUE OF ADMINISTRATION 25 

A CRITERION OF EFFECTIVENESS 

One criterion of effectiveness of technique of administration is the length 
of the stories. The average length of stories spoken by adults is three hundred 
words. The average for 10 year old children is one hundred and fifty words. 
According to Rapaport, the average story in the clinical situation is about one 
hundred words. Murray suggests that "stories from a sane adult averaging 
less than 140 words per story indicate lack of rapport and cooperation, lack 
of self-involvement. As a rule they are not worth scoring." This has to be 
qualified somewhat in terms of Henry's (40) experience with protocols from 
the Navaho and Hopi indians. The average story consisted of a few brief 
sentences, but the interpretations were nevertheless illuminating. 

Group administration of the test is practicable but systematic differences 
between results of group administration and individual administration have 
yet to be investigated. 



CHAPTER III 
THE SCORING SCHEME 

METHODOLOGICAL PROBLEMS 

In view of the complexity and scope of the material of TAT protocols, 
the dimensions of interpretation cannot differ in any appreciable degree from 
those necessary for personality analysis in general. A language of interpreta- 
tion adequate for all TAT protocols will, we venture, also be adequate for 
any type of personality analysis. Such a language is still to be achieved. What 
we will offer towards such a conceptual scheme can, in view of the stage of 
development of our science, be but a short step in that direction. 

More important than these suggestions for analysis is an understanding 
of the requirements which any such attempt must satisfy. In general, the 
variables of analysis must allow us to discover functional relationships of an 
ever increasing scope. The power of a set of variables might be described 
as an inverse relationship between the mass of empirical data and the mass of 
relating concepts. If we were to use for our analysis the language of the 
protocol itself, the possibility of generalization would be minimal. It is, more- 
over, essential that we employ variables which result in something more than 
taxonomic classifications. Description is the beginning but not the end of 
scientific inquiry, in which our highest aspiration has ever been the discovery 
of causal relationships. 

Criteria for inclusion and exclusion of concepts in such inquiry ultimately 
rest on the power of the complete set of variables rather than the power of any 
variable in isolation. Thus, in physical science, the concepts of mass and 
velocity taken independently might have yielded little; but mass, velocity, and 
a derivative of velocity, namely, acceleration, proved to be dimensions which 
allowed the discovery of general causal relationships. 

Perhaps the most important option we have in the selection of a set of 
categories for our purposes is the degree of generality and specificity of the 
concepts we employ. Consider first the dangers inherent in concepts of a high 
degree of generality. One such set of concepts is that employed by Murray 
in his general language for personality and TAT analysis his language of 
needs and press. Although the use of this language frequently yields incisive 
analysis of TAT protocol, there are nonetheless instances in which this lang- 
guage blurs specific aspects of a class of phenomena. Thus Renaud (74) 
reported, in connection with a study of head-injury cases and psychoneurotics, 
that he could find no significant difference between these groups with respect 
to any need or press described by Murray or Sanford. He did, however, find 

26 



THE SCORING SCHEME 2J 

that the length of the stories about picture u told by head-injury cases 
significantly exceeded that of the length of stories told by psychoneurotics.* 
On the basis of this difference in length, Renaud examined more closely the 
differences between the two sets of stories to this picture. He found that the 
stories of the head-injury cases were distinguished by a theme of the 
cataclysmic convergence of two opposing forces, of a last-ditch stand where 
the situation is either one of overpowering or being overpowered by aggressive 
force. He found the endings to this struggle varied; sometimes the hero 
killed the monster; in others the hero was killed by the monster. This is indeed 
press and need aggression according to the Murray-Sanford scoring scheme, 
but the summated score for the whole protocol has masked the significant 
difference which is elicited only in response to a remote other-world picture. 
We shall discuss this finding at some length in Chapter V, where we shall 
attempt to show that the degree of "remoteness" under which certain wishes 
are expressed is an index of the depth of repression. The general need "aggres- 
sion" does not distinguish these cases from psychoneurotic cases, but aggression 
qualified by the particular circumstances under which it may be expressed 
does. This is reflected in the differences in response to this single picture. 
Renaud's findings underline the danger of atomistic quantification and 
summing of responses according to concepts of too great generality. But the 
use of the Murray scoring scheme did elicit significant differences between 
head injury and brain disease cases on the one hand, and between psycho- 
neurotics and brain disease groups. We see then, in the case of a single 
investigation, at once both the strength and weakness of the same conceptual 
scheme. 

If it is possible to miss the critical, finer differentiations through the use 
of concepts which are too general, it is also possible that the same concept, 
too general in one instance, will be too specific in another context. Thus, in 
a study by the writer of an individual who was given the TAT before and 
during hypnosis, there was no statistically significant rise in the strength of 
either the need aggression or rejection, but if both of these needs were added 
together, the increase from the normal to hypnotic state was statistically 
significant. In other words, the needs rejection and aggression scored accord- 
ing to Murray's definitions were too specific to reveal the significant differences 
in this case. For this individual rejection was in fact another way of expressing 
aggression. 

If then, the same concept may in one case have precisely the degree of 
specificity required, in another case be too general, and in still another context 
be too specific, how shall we select our original dimensions of analysis? 

* Picture 1 1 shows a road skirting a deep chasm between high cliffs. On the road in 
the distance are obscure figures. Protruding from the rocky wall on one side is the long 
head and neck of a dragon. 



28 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

Our decision rests in part on the generality of our purpose. If our intent 
were to study the differences between normals and abnormals, we would 
probably have to use concepts of a higher order of generality than if we tried 
to delineate the differences between psychotics and neurotics. If, however, 
we wished to distinguish obsessive-compulsive neurotics from hysterics, we 
would probably need more specific categories; and even more highly differ- 
entiated catgories would be needed if our aim were to separate obsessive from 
compulsive neurotics. This is so because the data itself may vary in degree 
of generality or specificity. But, in general, when the TAT is used as an 
exploratory instrument, we do not know exactly what to look for. If we 
prejudge the categories of analysis, we may commit serious errors. What 
check then have we on the adequacy of our selection of categories of analysis ? 
It is our conviction that the logic of the individual's phantasy itself must be 
our ultimate criterion. Consider the following hypothetical examples. Let 
us suppose that in the stories of A we find the following sequences: (i) the 
hero injures another person outcome, the hero is unhappy; (2) the hero is 
angry with someone outcome, the hero is unhappy. In the stories of B 
(i) the hero injures another person outcome, the hero is unhappy; (2) the 
hero is angry with someone outcome, the hero gets over it and is happy. 
In the stories of C (i) the hero, helped by his gang, injures another person 
outcome, the hero is happy; (2) the hero injures another person outcome, 
the hero is unhappy. By using the individual's own phantasies, we are enabled 
to discover those differences in specificity and generality which "make a 
difference." For A, B, and C, we would have to employ different types of 
categories in the analysis of the aggressive need if we were to catch the causal 
relationships in these examples. In A's case, the concept "aggression" would 
be adequate, since the manifestation of any kind of aggression, whether felt 
covertly or overtly expressed, results in the same state of unhappiness. But in 
B's case, we would have to distinguish aggression according to the psycho- 
logical level on which it was expressed. By using this type of differentiation, 
we can show that the expression of overt aggression is one thing, as judged 
by one outcome, but that the feeling of anger means something else, since the 
consequences are different and depend on whether the feeling of anger is 
covertly or overtly expressed. In C's case, aggression would have to be specified 
by the context in which it was expressed; aggression expressed as a member 
of a group has a different meaning from aggression for which the individual 
assumes complete responsibility. This is but a sample of the different degrees 
of differentiation of "needs" in personality organization. But, though the 
varieties are great, the logic of the individual's stories do provide the dues 
necessary for the determination of those categories most adequate for the 
analysis of his protocol. We should employ those concepts which yield the 



THE SCORING SCHEME 29 

greatest degree of unification of the individual protocol. To the extent to 
which differences in the stories themselves can be explained, we have succeeded. 
Thus, had we used the concept "need aggression" in case C, we would have left 
unexplained the reason why the need aggression sometimes resulted in a good 
outcome, sometimes in a bad outcome. There are records which are recalcitrant 
to such analysis; but, in our experience, the principles of unification, if undis- 
covered, are testament more to the failure of the analysis than the poverty 
of the material. Not infrequently, this writer has all but given up the 
interpretation of a protocol, only to find the explanation some time later when 
the analysis was resumed. In the present state of our knowledge, an individual 
protocol may demand the same type of arduous exploration necessary for any 
frontier inquiry in science. It may be argued that the expenditure of such 
effort on a single case is unprofitable. It is the writer's conviction, however, 
that this attempt to extract lawfulness from the productions of the individual 
will ultimately yield uniformities of a broader scope. 

THE SCORING SCHEME 

Our solution to the problem of the set of concepts most likely to yield sig- 
nificant functional relationships from TAT stories is necessarily a tentative one. 
By its use we have been able to uncover relationships which otherwise escaped 
our attention. It must be admitted, however, that it suffers many of the 
inadequacies of other contemporary attempts in this direction. Its rationale 
consists in tapping varying levels of abstraction in the hope that significant 
aspects of diverse types of protocols will be detected by the use of concepts 
which range from a level of broad generality to a high degree of differentiation. 

In the writers* scoring scheme, each story is scored according to four main 
categories: vectors, levels, conditions, and qualifiers. 

VECTORS 

By vectors we mean the psychological direction characteristic of behavior, 
striving, wishes, cathexes, or feelings. We do not, in attributing direction, 
distinguish whether the behavior is a "need" or simply an act which is instru- 
mental to a "need." As we have differentiated them, they are not so general 
as the vectors employed either by Lewin (51) or Horney (43), or as specific 
as the needs differentiated by Murray (62).* Ten vectors are distinguished, 
their general meaning signified by a preposition. These vectors may have 
as their objects other persons, the self, social institutions, physical objects, ideas; 
in short, any object of any human interest. 

* The writer has borrowed much from Murray, deditferentiating his system of needs and 
press, and further differentiating other aspects of his conceptual scheme which it seemed 
useful to expand. 



JO THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

I. Vector "on" to depend upon objects. 

Examples: to seek or ask for help, admiration, recognition or in any 
way depend upon any object. To depend on the self, institutions, ideas, 
intuitions, etc. 

II. Vector "from" to acquire positive valences from objects. 

Examples: to earn or acquire money, possessions, to learn, to win love, 
to seduce, to eat, to learn from experience, etc. 

III. Vector "toward" to approach or enjoy objects of positive valence. 
Examples: to make friends, to make love, to experience esthetic enjoy- 
ment, to aspire to something, to love one's self, to sympathize or 
empathize with, etc. 

IV. Vector "with" to share experience with objects of positive valence. 
Examples: to play with, to have sexual experience with, to laugh 
with, to share intimacies, to work together, to commune with nature, to 
commune with self, etc. 

V. Vector "for" to bestow positive valence upon objects. 

Examples: to help another person, to repair objects, to create objects, 
to help the self, etc. 

VI. Vector "over" to govern objects. 

Examples: to dominate persons or situations, to overcome barriers, to 
continue or change a state, to drive an automobile, to control the self, etc. 
VIL Vector "under" to be governed by objects of negative valence. 

Examples: to comply, to surrender, to be superstitious of dreaded objects, 
to comply with an unwelcome super-ego or ego-ideal, etc. 
VEIL Vector "by" to be governed by objects of positive valence. 

Examples: to identify with a loved object, to be governed by lucky 
charms, to conform to an accepted super-ego or ego-ideal, etc. 

IX. Vector "away from" to avoid or escape or leave objects. 

Examples: to leave home, to avoid dangers, to escape harm, to avoid 
threats to pride, to suppress feelings, to forget unpleasant experiences, etc. 
X. Vector "against" to attack objects. 

Examples: to break the law, rebel, aggress upon others, to reject, to 
ridicule, to destroy, to criticize the self, or commit suicide. 

LEVELS 

By level we mean the plane of psychological function involved in the story. 
We have differentiated the following levels: 

I. Object description 

Example: the scene is a farmhouse. 

II. Events 

Example: it is his birthday. 

III. Behavior 

Example: he plowed the field. 

IV. Perception 

Example: he looked at the field. 
V. Attention 

Example: he listened for the sound. 



THE SCORING SCHEME 3! 

VI. Interest 

Example: he was curious. 
VII. Intention 

Example: he planned to do it. 
VIII. Sentiment 

Example: he was in favor of religion. 
IX. Thought 

Example: he pondered over the problem. 
X. Expectation 

Example: he thought it would rain. 
XL Wish 

Example: he wanted to do it. 
XII. Feeling, Mood 

Example: he was angry. 

he was depressed. 

XIII. Physical sensations 
Example: his arm hurt. 

XIV. Memory 

Example: he remembered his childhood. 
XV. Daydreams 

Example: he imagined himself as a great violinist 
XVI. Nightdreams 

Example: he had a nightmare. 
XVII. Special states 

Example: he was intoxicated. 

These levels may or may not have objects. Thus, the protocol may simply 
state: "This boy is daydreaming" or it may continue and tell what the day- 
dream is about. 

CONDITIONS 

By conditions, we mean any psychological, social, or physical state which 
is not itself behavior, striving, or wish. For example, if the hero has lost his 
parents, this is a "given," and is not his striving. Similarly, if the hero thinks 
he lacks ability, this is a condition with which he must cope, but, itself, is not 
his wish or striving. Inner states and feelings may also have this condition 
quality. The hero may be unhappy or in a state of depression (these would 
also, by our definition, be classified as conditions, since they are not in them- 
selves strivings or wishes). A condition may, however, be the object of a 
striving, as in the case of the hero who wishes to be happy or talented. We 
have distinguished the following conditions: 

I. States with negative valence. 
A. Lack 

Examples: to lack ability, skill, interest, feeling, knowledge, energy, 
control, persistence, courage, health, senses, success, love objects, sex 
objects, friends, advice, help, attention, sympathy, loyalty, parents, group 



32 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

membership, stimulation, money, possessions, free space, time, congenial 
environment, etc. 

B. Loss 

Examples: same object as under lack. 

C. Excess 

Examples: same objects as under lack but negatively cathected the hero 
is thought to have too many friends, too much money, too much ability, 
too much success, etc., for his own "good." 

D. Danger 

Examples: threats of lack, loss or excess, physical injury, drowning, 
fire, wild animals, etc. 

E. Structuration 

Examples: situations which are too well defined or not sufficiently 
defined, too unchanging, or too unstable. 

F. Inner states 

Examples: depression, anxiety, guilt, shame, uncertainty, conflict, 
disenchantment, etc. 
II. States with positive or neutral valence. 

A. Abundance 

Examples: same objects as under lack, except that these are possessed in 
abundance. 

B. Gratuities 

Examples: same objects as under abundance except that the individual 
is the passive recipient of valuable objects. 

C. Moderation 

Examples: same objects as under abundance, except that conditions are 
neither lacking nor abundant, but are sufficient. 

D. Security 

Examples: states free of any threat of danger, lack, loss, or excess. 

E. Structuration 

Examples: situations well defined, undefined, unchanging or unstable. 
These situations may be identical with those under negative states, but 
the hero finds them to his liking or accepts them. 

F. Inner states 

Examples: optimism, happiness, confidence, certainty, etc. 

QUALIFIERS 

By qualifiers we mean more specific aspects of either vectors, levels, or 
conditions. We distinguish the following qualifiers: 

I. Temporal characteristics: A. When does it happen? B. What is its duration? 

A. Past, present, or future. 
Immediate, distance, or remote. 

Examples: He loved her. He loves her. He will love her. He met her 
yesterday. He met her some months ago. He met her long, long ago. 

B. Transitory, medium duration, enduring. 

Examples: He loved her and left her. They knew each other for some 
time. He loved her all his life. 



THE SCORING SCHEME 33 

II. Contingency how certain is it? 

A. Possible, probable, certain, improbable, uncertain. 

Examples: He might do it. He will probably do it. He will do it. He 
will probably not do it. He doesn't know whether he will do it. 

III. Intensity what is its strength? 
A. Slight, medium, strong. 

Examples: He likes her. He loves her. He is desperately in love with 
her. 

IV. Negation any type of denial. This may be signified by parentheses around 
that which is denied. 

Examples: He didn't want to do it. It didn't happen. He did not 

have what he wanted. 

The statement, "He was not daydreaming," would be represented: 

(daydreaming) . 

V. Subsidiation any means-end relationship. This may be signified by the 
letter S following the behavior which is directed toward some end. 

Example: He made money in order to help his friends. (Vector "from" 

S Vector "for.") 

VI. Causality any causal relationship. This may be signified by the letter 
C following the cause. 

Example: His ability enabled him to succeed. Condition, Abundance 

C Condition, Abundance. 

In the use of this scheme, any variable may have any other variable as its 
object or it may have no object. Thus a vector may have as its object another 
vector, level, or condition, or any other object. Similarly, a level or condition 
may have any object. 

OBJECTS OF VECTORS 

Consider the possible objects of the vector "away from." An individual 
may wish to escape others or himself. He may wish to escape certain condi- 
tions, such as lack of money, a dull environment, or inner conflict. Or he 
may wish to escape certain levels, such as remembering, daydreaming, wishing, 
planning, acting, and so on. Or the object of this vector may be another vector, 
so that he wishes to escape helping or hurting someone (vector "away from," 
vector "for" or vector "against"). Or the vector may have no object, as in 
the statement, "He went away." 

OBJECTS OF LEVELS 

Levels may or may not have objects. If the protocol says, "The man is 
daydreaming," there may be no object of the level indicated in the story. 
Consider the level of thought. The object may be a vector, as in the state- 
ment, "He is thinking of leaving" (level thought; object vector "away 
from"). The object may be a condition, as in the statement, "He is thinking 
of the death of his friend" "(level thinking; object condition loss). Or the 



34 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

object may be another level, as in the statement, "He is thinking about his 
decision" (level thinking; object level intention). The object of a level 
may be another person. Thus in the statement, "He is thinking about his 
mother" the object of the level is "his mother." The object of the level may 
be the behavior of another person toward the hero, as in the statement, "He is 
thinking she will leave him," the object of the level is a person ("she") and 
the object of this person is the vector "away from," and the object of this 
vector is himself (level thinking; object she; object vector "away from"; 
object him). But a level may also have the characteristic of a qualifier, as 
in the statement, "He chopped wood," the level is behavior, which is a qualifier 
of vector "over"; object wood. 

OBJECTS OF CONDITIONS 

Conditions may or may not have objects. Thus, in the statement, "He 
felt there was something lacking," the object of the condition of lack may not 
be specified. But the condition may have as its object a vector, as in the 
statement, "He lacked self control," the object of the condition "lack" is the 
vector "over," object self. The condition may have as its object a level, as in 
the statement, "He lost his memory," the object of the condition "loss" is 
the level "memory." Or the condition may have another condition as its 
object, as in the statement, "He lost his confidence," the object of the condition 
"loss" is the condition "abundance." 

A SCORING SAMPLE 
Let us consider the scoring of the following story: 

The small boy hates to play his violin and his mother has made him practice 
because she hopes that some day he'll be a great musician. He gets so angry 
that he breaks the violin and then is sorry because he knows he'll get whipped 
and sure enough, he does 'cause it was a genuine Stradivarius. 

It is convenient in scoring a story to employ a large scoring sheet divided 
by ruled lines to allow space for every character and object mentioned in the 
story plus space for vectors, levels, conditions, and qualifiers. In this story 
there are two characters and one object. Three lines would be required for 
these, and four more lines for the scoring categories. 

Before scoring the story it is convenient to rewrite it so that temporal 
and functional relationships appear more clearly. We would rewrite the 
previous story as follows: 

Because she hopes that he'll be a great musician some day. 
His mother has made him practice. 
The small boy hates to play his violin. 



THE SCORING SCHEME 



35 



He gets so angry that 

He breaks the violin. 

And because he knows he'll get whipped 

Then is sorry. 

'Cause it was a genuine Stradivarius. 

Sure enough he does. 

This type of rewriting does some violence to the actual organization of 
the story and would obscure a study of its formal characteristics, but it is useful 
in exposing the story's dynamics. 

Let us turn now to the scoring of the rearranged story. Inasmuch as any 
category under which a sentence may be classified involves some distortion of 
its literal meaning, it is our practice to include with the categorization the 
exact words of the protocol on which the assignment of category has been 
based. This is done so that in the event the particular protocol calls for the 
employment of concepts of a level of generality different from that used in this 
particular scoring scheme, this new abstraction may be made more easily by 
inspection of the relation of the protocol to the assigned category. 

We shall illustrate the technique of scoring of this story, line by line. We 
shall first illustrate the general appearance of the scoring sheet followed by 
a running commentary on the rationale of the scoring. Let us examine the 
scoring sheet for the first sentence, "Because she hopes that hell be a great 
musician someday." 

i. Violin 1234 



2. Mother 

3. Son 

4. Vectors 

5. Levels 



6. Conditions 

7. Qualifiers 



Subject 
"she" 



Wish 
"hopes" 



Temporal 

immediate past 

Contingency 

certain 

Intensity 

medium 



Object 
"that he'll" 



Event 

"be a great 
musician" 

Abundance 

Temporal 
distant future 

"someday" 

Contingency 

certain 



C 

"because she 
hopes" 



36 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

In column i, row 2, we have written "subject," since it is the mother who 
is hoping her son will be a great musician. In row 5, the level is "wish," 
because "she hopes." In row 7, the temporal qualifier is "immediate past," 
because her wish slightly precedes the main action of the story, although its 
object is in the future. The contingency is certain, since there is no qualifica- 
tion to the activity of hoping. The protocol does not say "she might hope." 
The intensity is rated medium, since it does not appear unusually strong or 
weak. 

In column 2, row 3, we have written "object" because it is the mother's 
son who is the object of her hopes. 

In column 3, row 5, we have written "event," since, "being a great 
musician" makes no mention of behavior. In row 6 we have written 
"abundance," since being a "great" musician implies more than moderate 
achievement. In row 7, the temporal characteristic is "distant future," since 
it is hoped for "someday." But although it is put into the distant future, 
the contingency is certain, since the hope is that he "be" a great musician, 
not that he "might be" one. 

In column 4, row 7, we have written "C," indicating that because of 
the preceding conditions, what follows is a result. 

Let us analyze the next line of our rearranged story: "His mother has 
made him practice." This would be scored as follows: 

1 2 3 

1. Violin 

2. Mother Subject 

"his mother" 

3. Son Object 

"him" 

4. Vectors Over From 

"made him" "practice" 

5. Levels Behavior Behavior 

6. Conditions 

7. Qualifiers Temporal Temporal 

immediate past present 

"has made" 

Contingency 

certain 

Intensity 

medium 



THE SCORING SCHEME 37 

Column i, row 2, the mother is still the subject. In row 4, the vector is 
"over," since the mother "made" him practice. In row 5, the level is behavior, 
since the mother actually forces the child to do this. In row 7, the temporal 
qualifier is "immediate past," since it slightly precedes the present situation as 
described in the remainder of the story. The contingency is certain, since 
it has actually occurred. The intensity is medium. 

In column 2, row 3, the object is the son, who is made to practice. 

In column 3, row 4, the vector is "from," since the son has been forced to 
practice. Implicit is the vector "under," since this represents being governed 
by negatively cathected objects, but we do not score this, since the sentence 
tells us only that the mother forced him to practice his compliance is not 
mentioned directly. In row 5, the level is "behavior" since he has been made 
to do something. In row 7, the temporal qualifier is "present." 

Let us next analyze the sentence, "The small boy hates to play his violin." 

* 2 3 

i. Violin Object 

"play his violin" 

* 2. Mother 

3. Son Subject 

"the small boy" 

4. Vectors Against From 

"hates" "to play" 

5. Levels Feeling Behavior 

6. Conditions 

7. Qualifiers Temporal Temporal 

present present 

Contingency 

certain 
Intensity 
medium 

In column i, row 3, the subject is the son. In row 4, the vector is 
"against," since he "hates" to play his violin. In row 5, the level is "feeling," 
since it is a feeling of hatred rather than overt aggressive behavior. In row 7, 
the temporal qualifier is present, since his hatred is described as a consequence 
of the immediate past coercion by his mother. The contingency is "certain," 
and the intensity would be considered "medium," since it is only later that 
this feeling grows more intense. 

In column 2, row 4, the vector is "from," since the object of his hatred 



38 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

is not the violin, but the playing of it. In row 5, the level is clearly behavioral. 
In row 7, the temporal qualifier is still the present. 

In column 3, row i, the object of the vector "from" is the violin which is 
what he must play. The object of the feeling of hatred is the vector "from 
playing" and the object of the vector "from" is the object "violin." 

Let us turn next to the sentence, "He gets so angry that." 



1. Violin 

2. Mother 

3. Son Subject 

"He" 

4. Vectors Against 

"gets so angry" 

5. Levels Feeling 

6. Conditions 

7. Qualifiers Temporal C 

present "that" 

Contingency 

certain 

Intensity 

strong 

In column i, row 3, the subject is still the son. In row 4 the vector is 
"against," since he "gets so angry." In row 5, the level is still that of feeling. 
In row 7, the temporal qualifier is still the extended present, the contingency 
is certain and the intensity has increased to strong, since he says "so" angry. 

In column 2, row 7, t the qualifier is causality, since he says "that," implying 
that what follows is a consequence of his growing anger. 

Let us analyze next the line, "He breaks the violin." 

i 2 

1. Violin Object 

"breaks the violin" 

2. Mother 

3. Son Subject 

"he" 

4. Vectors Against 

"breaks the violin" 

5. Levels Behavior 



THE SCORING SCHEME 39 



6. Conditions 

7. Qualifiers Temporal 

present 

Contingency 

certain 

Intensity 

medium 

In column i, row 3, the subject is still the son. In row 4, the vector is 
"against" because he breaks the violin and, for the same reason, on row 5 
the level is now behavior. On row 7, the temporal qualifier is still the 
extended present, the contingency remains certain, but the intensity of the 
behavior is rated as medium, since there is no reference to the intensity of 
the behavior, although there had been mention of intense feelings of anger. 
If there had been elaboration of his destructive behavior, with mention of 
breaking it into little pieces, etc., it would have been rated strong in intensity. 
In column 2, row i, the object of this destruction is the violin. 

Let us analyze next the line, "And because he knows he'll get whipped." 

i 2 3 

1. Violin 

2. Mother 

3. Son Subject Subject 

"he" "he'll" 

4. Vectors Under 

"get whipped** 

5. Levels Expectation Behavior 

"knows* 1 

6. Conditions 

7. Qualifiers Temporal Temporal C 

present immediate future "because . . . then'* 

Contingency Contingency 

certain certain 

In column i, row 3, the subject continues to be the son. In row 5, the 
level is expectation, since he "knows" and expects to get whipped. In row 7, 
the temporal qualifier is still the extended present, the contingency certain. 

In column 2, row 3, the subject of the expectation is himself. In row 4, 
the vector is "under," since he allows himself to be governed by a negatively 
cathected object. In row 5, the level is behavioral, since he knows he will 
behave submissively. In row 7, the temporal qualifier is the immediate futifre, 



4 o 



THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 



indicated by the word "he'll," and the contingency is certain, since he does not 
say he'll probably get whipped. 

In column 3, row 7, the qualifier is causality, since he says "because he 
knows . . , then." 

The next line to be analyzed is "then is sorry." 



i. Violin 






2. Mother 






3. Son 


Subject 




4. Vectors 






5. Levels 




Feeling 
"is sorry" 


6. Conditions 




Negative inner state 


7. Qualifiers 


Temporal 
immediate future 
"then" 


Temporal 
immediate future 
Intensity 
medium 



In column i, row 3, the subject remains the son. In row 7, the temporal 
qualifier is the immediate future, indicated by the word "then." 

In column 2, row 5> the level is feeling "is sorry," which is a negative 
inner state, marked on row 6. On row 7, the temporal qualifier is the imme- 
diate future, and the intensity is medium. 

The next sentence to be analyzed is, " 'Cause it was a genuine Stradivarius." 



1. Violin 

2. Mother 

3. Son 

4. Vectors 

5. Levels 

6. Conditions 

7. Qualifiers 



i 

Subject 
"it" 



Object description 

'Vas a genuine 

Stradivarius" 

Abundance 
"genuine" 



C 

"cause it was" 



THE SCORING SCHEME 4! 

In column i, row i, the subject is the violin. On row 5, the level is 
"object description." In row 6, the condition is abundance, since the violin 
is characterized as a valuable one. In column 2, row 7, the qualifier is 
causality, since the value of the violin is the reason for what follows, " 'Cause 
it was a genuine Stradivarius." 

The final line to be analyzed is, "Sure enough he does." 

i 

1. Violin 

2. Mother 

3. Son Subject 

"he" 

4. Vectors Under 

"does" 

5. Levels Behavior 

6. Conditions 

7. Qualifiers Temporal 

immediate future 

Contingency certain 

"sure enough he does" 

In column i, row 3, the subject is still the son. In row 4, the vector is 
"under," since he submits to a whipping, and, by the same token, the level, 
on row 5, is behavioral. On row 7, the temporal qualifier is the immediate 
future, and the contingency is certain "sure enough he does." 

This sample of the application of the scoring scheme may well discourage 
the reader from ever attempting to score the TAT. The busy clinician will 
certainly doubt the practicability of such time-consuming microscopic analysis. 
But we would nonetheless argue its usefulness. One or more perusals of a 
protocol may reward the experienced interpreter and yield significant and 
accurate diagnoses of personality. But, in the writer's experience, it is no less 
frequent a phenomenon that repeated perusals of a single protocol may yield 
little of significance and less of certainty. In such cases, continued effort 
sometimes rewards the labors of the interpreter; and one may be astonished 
at the simplicity of the functional relationships which had somehow escaped 
scrutiny for so long. In our experience, however, systematic analysis, though 
laborious and time-consuming, may be less time-consuming and more effective 
than endless perusal of a protocol recalcitrant to intuition. After completing 
such a microscopic analysis, rapid breakdowns may be made on the stories. 
One may consider outcomes, inner states such as anxiety, vectors such as 
aggression, and determine relatively quickly what are the specific patterns 
of conditions which produce, or result from, these variables. This cannot be 



42 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

done so easily by inspection of the protocol, since a single sentence may 
contain "hidden" variables which may be explicitly recognized only after 
careful analysis of the elements of the story. 

We would also argue its usefulness as an instrument of training. Refined 
discrimination, whether it be in microscopy, crime detection, or music apprecia- 
tion, is necessarily a hard-won achievement. Befoie one may abstract general 
relationships of scope and power, the elements entering into these relationships 
must be differentiated. 

But perhaps the most important consideration is the necessity at this time 
for some consensus of meaning on the part of those utilizing this test. Such 
consensus cannot be put off indefinitely if the test is to be useful. It is the 
writer's hope that the suggested conceptual scheme may be a step in this 
direction.* 

SUMMARY 

We have examined some of the requirements which any scoring scheme 
for the TAT must satisfy. Most important, we held, was the requirement 
that the set of dimensions employed enable us to discover functional relation- 
ships of an ever increasing scope. The first option concerned the degree of 
generality or specificity of concepts to be used. We saw that a concept which 
was useful in one protocol might be too general for another protocol and yet 
too specific in still another one. We suggested that the ultimate criterion in 
the initial selection of concepts was the logic of the individual protocol that 
we should employ concepts of that degree of generality or specificity which 
matched the level of the protocol we were analyzing. Since differentiations 
which are crucial in one case make no difference in another, our suggested 
scoring scheme was based on the rationale of tapping as wide a range of levels 
of generality as possible, Thus, by the use of qualifiers, concepts which may 
be too general for many protocols are made more specific through further 
qualification. Flexibility and economy of language were sought by assigning 
variables a twofold function, that of subject or object, so that any variable 
might be either the subject or object of any other variable. We differentiated 
four general classes of variables. Vectors were defined as the psychological 
directions characteristic of behavior, striving, wishes, cathexes or feelings. 
Level referred to the plane of psychological function involved in the story. 
Conditions were defined as those "given" states, psychological, social, or 
physical, which are not behavior, striving, or wish. Qualifiers were defined 
as the more specific aspects of vectors, levels, or conditions. 

*The writer has not however systematically employed the language of this scoring 
scheme in the text, since he believes that the utilization of the TAT should not be inextri- 
cably linked with any particular language o analysis. Implicit in many of the techniques 
of analysis presented in this text, however, are the concepts employed in this scoring scheme. 
The writer has striven to translate these concepts into common English wherever possible. 



CHAPTER IV 

INTRODUCTION TO THE TECHNIQUE OF 
INTERPRETATION 

After identification and scoring of the variables of each story is com- 
pleted we are at the threshold of interpretation. "Interpretation" has, in psycho- 
analytic doctrine, lost some of its pristine meaning. Interpretation is an activity 
common to all science. Unless it is our assumption that the interpretation 
of psychological data necessitates principles of inference peculiar to psychology, 
we must adopt the methodology common to all inductive and deductive science. 
It is our purpose in this chapter to demonstrate the applicability of long- 
accepted canons of inference to picture-provoked phantasy. Let us consider 
first the method of agreement of John Stuart Mill (60). 

A. METHOD OF AGREEMENT 
Let us consider the relationship between the following stories.* 

1. This boy has just been given a violin and he's delighted with it. He wants 
to become a great violinsr. He is encouraged by his parents. He works hard 
and eventually he becomes a great violinst. 

2. This boy is very unhappy because he has no friends. His parents encourage 
him to make friends. He still has no friends until one day a new boy moves 
into the neighborhood. The new boy asks him to play with him and 
eventually they become good friends. 

These two stories may be analyzed according to Mill's method of agree- 
ment, "If two or more instances of the phenomenon under investigation have 
only one circumstance in common, the circumstance in which alone all the 
instances agree is the cause, or effect of the given phenomenon." Let the 
given phenomenon be the successful outcome. In every condition which 
precedes the outcome, the encouragement by the parents is the only common 
condition. Thus, in i he has been given a violin; in 2 he lacks friends. In 
i he is happy; in 2 unhappy. In i the region is work and achievement; 
in 2 it is the social region. In i he works hard; in 2 he is passive in the face 
of social isolation. In i the means to eventual success and happiness is 
through hard work; in 2 it is through a gratuitous event for which he has 

*The stories have been edited in this chapter, for didactic purposes. The number of 
the picture to which stories have been told has been omitted throughout, partly because 
many of the stories have been told to pictures not included in the present set of TAT cards 
but primarily because the principal techniques of interpretation employed in this book are 
not based on the relationship between the picture and the story. 

43 



44 



THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 



not worked. The outcome the gratification of the original wish may be 
said to be the effect of the encouragement of his parents, since this alone is 
what the two instances have in common. This type of analysis is particularly 
illuminating in this case because in neither story is the encouragement of the 
parents causally related by the storyteller to the final outcome. The individual 
may not, in his imaginative productions, know, or clearly tell us what is 
cause and what is effect. 

B, METHOD OF DIFFERENCE 
Consider now the relationship between these stories. 

1. This is someone who has just lost a person very dear to him. He is in 
the depths of despair. Life has lost all meaning for him and he doesn't want 
to go on living. But then he meets a woman who understands die way he 
feels and gradually he forgets his sorrow and he finds again that there is 
meaning in life. 

2. This is the picture of a man who is mourning someone who was very 
close to him. Nothing seems to matter very much to him since her death. 
He sees no more point in living. Death comes as a welcome relief from his 
misery. 

The relationship between these stories illustrates Mill's method of dif- 
ference. "If an instance in which the phenomenon under investigation occurs, 
and an instance in which it does not occur, have every circumstance in common, 
save one, that one occurring only in the former, the circumstance in which 
alone the two instances differ is the effect, or cause, or an indispensable part 
of the cause of the phenomenon." Thus, in i and 2, there is the loss of a love 
object, and a consequent depression. But in i there is the intervention of a 
new love object who understands him; and the outcome is regeneration. In 2, 
there is no new love object, and the outcome is a continuation of the depression 
to its end in death. Thus, we may say that recovery from depression is the 
effect of the intervention of another love object. 

C. JOINT METHOD OF AGREEMENT AND DIFFERENCE 

Let us reconsider now the protocol of the first case, adding two more 
stories from the same protocol. 

1. This boy has just been given a violin, and he's delighted with it. He wants 
to become a great violinist He is encouraged by his parents. He works hard 
and eventually he becomes a great violinist. 

2. This boy is very unhappy because he has no friends. His parents encourage 
him to make friends. He still has no friends until one day a new boy moves 
into the neighborhood. The new boy asks him to play with him and eventually 
they become good friends. 



TECHNIQUE OF INTERPRETATION 45 

i'. He has just been given a raise. He wants more than anything to become 
the head of the firm, and he works very, very hard, but somehow he never 
makes the grade. 

2'. This fellow is very unhappy because he doesn't have any girl friends. He 

mopes and mopes. One night when he is feeling very lonely a girl picks him 

up and after that he sees quite a lot of her. But eventually she leaves him 
and he is lonely again. 

This set of stones may be analyzed according to Mill's joint method of 
agreement and difference. "If two or more instances in which the phenomenon 
occurs have only one circumstance in common, while two or more instances 
in which it does not occur have nothing in common save the absence of that 
circumstance, the circumstance in which alone the two sets of instances differ 
is the effect or the cause or an indispensable part of the cause, of the phe- 
nomenon." Since one can never be certain that either the method of agree- 
ment or difference is being perfectly applied, the joint method strengthens the 
evidence. It must be rememberd that there are always similarities and dif- 
ferences between stones which may be unrecognized by the interpreter. 

Here i and i' are similar and 2 and 2' are similar, but not exactly alike. 
Thus, in i and i', the age of the hero is different; in 2 the region is social, 
whereas in 2' it is the love region which is described. Otherwise, however, 
the fit is very close. In both i and i' the hero is given something; the hero 
aspires to great achievement and works hard for it. The difference is pri- 
marily in the success of the outcome and the absence of parental encourage- 
ment in i'. In 2 and 2', both heroes are unhappy because of the lack of 
cathected objects both are rescued by more active individuals and this leads 
to friendship. The difference, again, is primarily in the success of the outcome 
and the absence of parental encouragement in 2'. The evidence for the neces- 
sity of parental support if the hero is to achieve his ends is thereby strengthened. 
Just as we can never be certain in experimental work that all factors have 
been controlled, we rarely find in TAT protocols the homogeneity of con- 
ditions which would allow unambiguous interpretation of differences between 
stories. For this reason the joint method is to be preferred wherever it is 
possible to employ it. 

D. METHOD OF CONCOMITANT VARIATION 

Frequently, stories do not involve the complete presence or absence of 
certain effects or causes but, rather, variations in degree. This is the case 
in the following stones. 

i. He has had a hard time of it. He has never had much money, and has 
always had to work hard. He manages to make a living, but he is not a 
great success. 



46 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

2. This is a fellow who has struggled all his life. He has never had the 
money he wanted, but he worked very hard. When others took vacations, 
he would work all the harder. After years of backbreaking effort, he became 
a millionaire. 

Here we may employ Mill's method of concomitant variation. "What- 
ever phenomenon varies in any manner whenever another phenomenon varies 
in some particular manner, is either a cause or an effect of that phenomenon, 
or is connected with it through some fact of causation." In i and 2, we find 
that the degree of success varies concomitantly with the degree of effort, all 
other factors being equal. These stories do not reveal what are the causes of 
this difference in effort, but the relationship between effort and success is 
clear. The harder he works, the more successful he is. 

E. Two FACTORS, BOTH NECESSARY AND SUFFICIENT CAUSES 

These methods of Mill take us part of the way. Mill's "phenomenon" 
is, in psychological matters, rarely a single factor, and the cause of an effect 
usually involves gestalten. Let us, then, expand the inquiry to cope with 
causes or effects which involve at least two factors. 

Let us consider the relationship between these four stories. 

1. This man is in a state of indecision. He is wondering what he should do 
in his business. His father advises him what he should do. He works hard 
and becomes a success. 

2. He is wondering what to do. His father gives him advice and shows him 
how to run his business. He comes down to his store and helps out, but the 
business flops. 

3. He is in a quandry. He doesn't know what to do with his business. His 
father says, "I told you you'd never make a go of it." He works hard to 
save it, but he goes bankrupt. 

4. This man is facing bankruptcy, and his father is berating him for his 
stupidity. He becomes more and more depressed, and his business goes 
downhill steadily till it fails. 

In these stories we have an example of the case in which two factors 
are both necessary and sufficient causes of the phenomenon "success." Where, 
in i there is both advice from the father and hard work by the hero, the hero 
becomes a success. But, in 2, the hero is both instructed and helped by the 
father. There is no mention, however, of the hero's efforts, and the outcome 
is failure. In 3, he works hard, but he is criticized rather than advised, and 
the business fails. In 4, there is again criticism which depresses the hero 
to such an extent that he does not try to save the business, and the outcome 
is again failure. It appears that neither the father's advice, nor the son's hard 



TECHNIQUE OF INTERPRETATION 47 

work is sufficient to guarantee a successful outcome if one is present and the 
other absent. When the father's advice is followed by actual help and inter- 
vention in his son's enterprise, there is no mention that the son works. It 
would seem that advice is necessary, but it must not go beyond advice or the 
son's initiative will be stifled. The son's hard work alone will not guarantee 
success; presumably his efforts must be guided to some extent, or the effort 
is in vain. 

Let us generalize this type of analysis and call paternal advice x and the 
hero's own work y. If we are to assert that both x and y are necessary and 
sufficient causes of phenomenon P, it is necessary to show that, when x and y 
appear, P is a consequence and that when only x appears, as in 2, P does not 
appear and when only y appears, as in 3, P does not appear and, when neither 
x nor y appears, as in 4, P does not appear. In other words, for both x and y 
to be necessary and sufficient causes, the phenomenon must appear as a con- 
sequent only if x and y appear. This type of relationship is of critical impor- 
tance in the diagnosis of personality for therapeutic or guidance purposes. In 
the treatment of anxiety or depression we not infrequently find that the patho- 
logical reaction is incited or sustained by a combination of factors, neither of 
which occurring separately is pathogenic. Using this knowledge, if we can 
reduce either factor the reaction may be controlled. Similarly, an anxiety state 
may be sustained by the absence of two or more factors. Therapy of such 
conditions calls for the introduction of a combination of factors and if we 
were to employ only one factor at a time the condition would not yield. Thus, 
in a case similar to that above, TAT analysis revealed that success depended 
on paternal advice which did not stifle initiative. Wherever this advice was 
extended to the point of assuming some of the individual's responsibilities, the 
patient experienced anxiety. Wherever this individual worked without such 
advice, there was also anxiety. In his past history, the only period free of 
anxiety occurred when he was working for a paternalistic scientist on a govern- 
ment project. The latter helped him but allowed him considerable initiative 
in the solution of his problems. On leaving this environment, this individual 
developed acute anxiety attacks. Rapport in the diagnostic and therapeutic 
situation hung on a delicate balance between a directive and nondirective 
attitude on the part of the therapist. If the therapist veered too much in one 
direction or the other, it precipitated acute anxiety. The therapist was able 
to reduce substantially the patient's anxiety by maintaining the attitude which 
the TAT indicated was necessary for the patient's adequate functioning. 
Eventually he was again willing to continue the career which he had relin- 
quished under the pressure of his anxiety attacks. 

Similar problems arise in marital relationships. Individuals who need 
both support and respect may experience acute frustration in marriage if 



48 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

either element is missing from the relationship. The same joint needs will be 
found characteristic of some individuals in the work setting. Others are able 
to split the satisfaction of these two needs, requiring support from their wives 
and respect from their business associates. This type of analysis of TAT 
protocols yields insight into the precise pattern of conditions necessary for 
adequate functioning in a variety of regions. 

Though asserting that two factors are both necessary and sufficient condi- 
tions for a phenomenon, we have not specified the exact relationship between 
these two factors. It is a synergistic relationship, so that success depends 
neither on hard work nor on advice but on the pattern of hard work which 
is guided. It cannot represent a chain of causal connections where, for example, 
advice leads to hard work which in turn produces success. The case in which 
there is hard wor\ without success, i.e., story 3, precludes the possibility that 
advice alone is sufficient cause for hard work, since the hero works without 
advice. It also precludes the possibility that hard work is sufficient cause of 
success, since hard work results here in failure. 

F. EITHER OF Two FACTORS BUT NOT BOTH AS SUFFICIENT CAUSE 

Let us consider another type of causal relationship, exemplified in the 
stories which follow, 

1. This fellow is having a fight with the girl. They have been in love for 
years and now he is telling her that he no longer loves her and that they 
are through. He leaves her and feels better than he has in years. 

2. These are two lovers. They are very deeply in love, and they are sharing 
the intimacy which only those who have lived together for so long are capable 
of. These are the moments they both live for. 

3. This man is in a great conflict He has been in love with this woman for 
a long time, but there is something about her which he can't stand. They 
were to be married, but he cannot bring himself to marry her or let her go. 
They go on for years this way. He wonders why he can't be happy with her 
or without her. 

4. This fellow is lonely and miserable. He is in a strange town and is on 
the prowl. But all the women have escorts and he goes back to his hotel 
room more miserable than ever. 

Here we see the instance in which one or the other of two factors is a 
sufficient cause of a phenomenon, but not both together. This individual tells 
us that he will be happy if he rejects a love object, or accepts a love object, 
but that he will be unhappy if he both accepts and rejects a love object, or has 
no love objects. Generalizing these relationships, let us call love x and 
rejection y. To assert that either x or y is a sufficient condition of P, but not 
both x and y together, we must show that, when x appears, the resultant is P; 



TECHNIQUE OF INTERPRETATION 49 

when y appears, the resultant is P; when x and y appear, P does not appear; 
and that P does not appear under any other circumstances than x or y. 
In i, y, rejection of the love object, produces P, a happy outcome. In 2, x, the 
acceptance of the love object, produces P, a happy outcome. In 3, the simul- 
taneous acceptance x and rejection y of the love object does not produce P, 
a happy outcome. In the absence of either x, the acceptance of a love object, 
or y, the rejection of a love object, in story 4, the quest of a love object does 
not produce a happy outcome. 

In this type of relationship, the simultaneous operation of two factors 
detracts from the efficacy characteristic of each independent factor. In con- 
trast to the relationship in which both factors are necessary and sufficient, 
here each factor does stand in a means-end relationship to the resultant phe- 
nomenon. It is not a means-end chain between x, y, and P, but either x 01 y 
may be a means to the same end (P). In these stories, the hero can be happy 
whether he loves or hates, but he cannot be happy if he has no object to love 
or hate or if he both loves and hates the same object. 

The paralyzing consequences of two antithetical forces need no under- 
lining. But the implications of this type of relationship are broader than the 
problem of psychological conflict and ambivalence. Thus, in the work region, 
let us suppose that x is a skeptical attitude on the part of the individual's 
employer and that y represents work of a difficult nature. There are protocols 
in which an individual may be challenged sufficiently by either the difficulty 
of the work or by the skeptical attitude of his employer to work well and 
achieve success. But the combination of difficult work and a skeptical attitude 
on the part of the employer may produce passivity rather than counteractive 
effort. The TAT thus affords us, through this type of analysis, an insight 
into the frustrations which elicit the best energies of the individual and a 
delineation of the specific conditions under which the load may become too 
great. The same technique of analysis may be used to determine the degree 
of rejection which spurs a person to win a love object, and the concatenation 
of barriers which produces withdrawal. Again, by this technique we may 
analyze the general type of threat which produces heroic struggle and the 
cumulative dangers which result in abject surrender. 

G. EITHER OR BOTH OF Two FACTORS AS SUFFICIENT CAUSE 
Another type of causal relationship is exemplified in the following stories: 

1. The older man has just given the younger man a pep talk. The young man 
goes to work with new enthusiasm. He does his work so well that he gets 
a raise. 

2. This man has a problem. The other fellow shows him what he knows 
and he is such an apt student that before long he is given more responsibility. 



50 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

3. He seems unhappy and discouraged. Then somebody comes along and asks 
him what the trouble is. He tries to make him feel better, and shows him 
what he's been doing wrong. In no time at all he does feel better, and he 
sees that all he needed was a little know-how, and he can do it as well if 
not better than the next fellow. 

4. This guy is in a mess. He's only been working a few days and he doesn't 
know exactly what to do, and his foreman is always bawling him out, and 
yelling at him to keep working. He does keep working, but at the end of the 
week when he gets his pay check there is a slip in it telling him that his 
services are no longer required. 

In these stones we have an example of the case in which cither or both 
of two factors is a sufficient cause of the phenomenon "success." Thus in i, 
encouragement leads to success. In 2 instruction results in success. In 3 both 
encouragement and instruction produce success. But in 4, neither encourage- 
ment nor instruction is offered, and the outcome is failure. Generalizing, 
let x represent encouragement and y represent instruction. If we are to assert 
that either x or y, or both, is a sufficient cause of phenomenon P, we would 
have to show that P appeared when x appeared or when y appeared or when 
x and y appeared but did not appear if neither x nor y appeared. These stories 
satisfy these criteria; a successful outcome follows either encouragement or 
instruction or both. When neither encouragement nor instruction appears, 
the outcome is unsuccessful. 

In this type of relationship, x and y are, as above, means to the same 
end, but they do not interfere with each other in the production of this end. 
They may result in precisely the same phenomenon or their action together 
may be synergistic. Thus encouragement and instruction may result in pre- 
cisely the same degree of productivity, or in a greater degree than if either 
were present alone. In the examples above, there was no synergistic interaction. 
This type of analysis is of diagnostic importance in vocational guidance when 
it is important to know whether an individual requires both instruction and 
encouragement; or one or the other but not both; or one or the other or both. 

H. MULTIFACTOR ANALYSIS 

We have examined some of the techniques for analyzing two factors as 
cause or effect. Let us turn now to the problem of multif actor analysis. We 
have seen that what appears to be a cause and effect relationship within any 
single story may in the light of other stories prove to be something quite dif- 
ferent. It is clear that we cannot interpret any single story without considering 
other stories which involve related factors. 

Let us consider a hypothetical story in which a hero who has worked 
hard all his life loses his job, becomes pessimistic and commits suicide. We 



TECHNIQUE OF INTERPRETATION 51 

shall attempt to show that this single story provides no basis whatever for 
interpretation, that it can be interpreted only on the basis of the concomitant 
variation of the elements of this story with related elements in other stories. 
We will now examine the varieties of meanings which may be expressed by 
this story, as an example of the interpretation of any story. 

I. THE STORY AS AN EXPRESSION OF THE INDIVIDUAL'S VALUES 

The story may be an expression of the individual's values. Suppose that 
our hypothetical story meant that work was the individual's central value. 
If this were the case we would find that in other stories which mentioned 
threats to other values disruption of love, family or social relationships the 
hero was not seriously disturbed but that the hero was seriously disturbed in 
any story in which his work was threatened. 

On the other hand, suppose that both work and love were the individual's 
central values. If this were so, stories involving threats to either of these 
values would be disturbing; threats to any other value, however, would be 
less serious. 

2. THE STORY AS AN EXPRESSION OF THREAT 

The story may express threats with which the individual is concerned. 
Thus our story might mean that the individual was peculiarly sensitive and 
vulnerable to the threat of loss. If this were the case there would be other 
stories in which there were losses of many kinds which also resulted In 
pessimism and suicide losses of love objects, friends, parents or physical* 
objects. In stories in which he suffered no loss, the hero would be optimistic 
and the outcome happy. 

Or this story might be an example of a general fear of any kind of 
frustration rather than fear of the specific threat of loss. If this were the 
case we would find other stories expressing a variety of frustrations of many 
values, all leading to pessimism and suicide. Stories in which the hero suf- 
fered no frustration would end more happily. 

Or the story may be expressing the individual's differential reaction to 
one type of threat. Thus it might mean that he can tolerate any state of lack, 
but not a state of loss. If this were the case there would be other stories 
involving losses of all types which resulted in pessimism and suicide, but in 
stories where the hero lacked the same objects, he would overcome the deficit. 

Or the story might express the individual's differential reaction to losses 
of varying magnitude. If this were the case there would be other stories 
in which losses of less serious magnitude resulted in less serious disturbance. 
The hero in this story loses his job and commits suicide; in another story he 
might lose a week's pay through illness, be distressed, but not commit suicide. 



52 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

Or he might in one story lose his wife's affection and commit suicide and in 
another lose the affection of a girl friend and be distressed but not commit 
suicide. 

3. THE STORY AS AN EXPRESSION OF INVARIANCE 

Any story may express an invariant characteristic of the individual which 
is reflected in invariant repetitions of some element throughout the stories. 

Thus this story might mean that the individual expects to be unsuccessful 
in whatever he does. If this were the case we would find that all his stones, 
whatever the nature of the wish or striving, involve failure. 

It might, however signify that he expects to be unsuccessful only in his 
work. If this were the case we would find that all other stories involving 
work end in failure but whenever any other striving is involved the hero is 
successful. 

Or the story might be an expression of the individual's general pessimism 
rather than his expectation of failure. If such were the case, all heroes would 
be pessimistic whether they succeed or fail and whether the theme of the story 
is concerned with work or another region. 

If the story signified that the hero is pessimistic only with respect to 
work, all other stories involving work would describe a pessimistic hero, but 
the hero would be optimistic in stories involving other regions. 

The story might indicate a basic self destructive wish. If this were the 
case all other stories would involve a hero who ultimately destroyed himself. 

But if this story meant that the hero had such wishes only in connection 
with his work, there would be other stories in which work leads to suicide 
but no suicide in any other type of story. 

4. THE STORY AS AN EXPRESSION OF NECESSARY CONDITIONS 

Any story may be an expression of the conditions necessary for any element 
within the story. Thus this story might be an expression of the conditions 
necessary for the hero's success or failure in work. If this were the case other 
stories in which the hero worked hard all his life would also end in failure, 
but there would be stories of heroes who did not lose their jobs and in these 
we would find some preceding condition other than hard work. Such heroes 
might, for example, have worked for employers who were interested in them. 

The story might also be an expression of the conditions that cause the 
individual to become pessimistic. If this were the case there would be other 
stories in which the loss of his job made the hero pessimistic and stories in 
which the hero was prevented by his wife's sympathy, for example from 
becoming pessimistic over the loss of his job. 

Finally, the story may define the conditions which would incite the indi- 



TECHNIQUE OF INTERPRETATION 53 

vidual to commit suicide. If this were the case the hero would take his life 
in situations similar in any way to this one, but he would not commit suicide 
if conditions were different in any way if, for example, he did not lose his 
job or if he were not pessimistic following such loss, or if someone encouraged 
him after he had become pessimistic. An examination 6f the conditions which 
did not lead to suicide in other stones would enable us to -determine which 
of the conditions in this story did lead to suicide whether it was the loss of 
his job or the pessimism which followed this. If the loss of his job were 
responsible for his suicide there would be other stories in which the hero, 
having lost his job, commits suicide. If, however, it was pessimism that 
caused him to take his life, he would not commit suicide in other stories in 
which pessimism did not follow the loss of his job. 

We have attempted to demonstrate the varieties of interpretation possible 
in any single story and the technique of analysis used to discover the specific 
meaning which may be inherent in the story. Such analysis necessarily involves 
a consideration of the total protocol and, as we shall illustrate in a later chapter, 
may involve information which must be sought outside the TAT. 

SUMMARY 

We have examined seven techniques commonly employed in the inter- 
pretation of any type of scientific data. We have suggested that the interpreta- 
tion of TAT stories may employ canons of inference long accepted by other 
sciences. These were seen to be "models" to which empirical data might be 
fitted with varying degrees of error. Since the presence of unrecognized factors 
is an inherent characteristic of empirical inquiry, the employment of these 
methods cannot guarantee valid inferences, but will reduce some of the error 
which might otherwise enter into our interpretations. 

As applied to TAT stories, Mill's method of agreement require two stories 
in which a common effect is preceded by only one other common condition. 
This common condition is then the cause of the common effect. In our 
examples we chose common outcomes as effects, but any element in the story 
may be analyzed in the same manner. According to the method of difference, 
two stories must be similar in every respect but one; this difference is then 
the cause of the difference in outcome. In the joint method of agreement and 
difference, both methods are employed and the evidence is thereby strengthened. 
There must be two stories with only one common condition leading to the 
same effect and two sets of stories in which each set of two is similar in every 
respect but one and with differences in outcomes which are the result of the 
dissimilar causes. In the method of concomitant variation, differences in 



54 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

degree of an effect are related to concomitant variation in differences in degree 
of the causal condition. 

Inasmuch as most psychological phenomena involve a pattern of factors 
as cause and effect, we expanded the inquiry to cope with at least two factors. 
Where two factors are both necessary and sufficient causes it is necessary to 
show that the effect must follow only if both factors appear. If either one, or 
neither, is present, the effect cannot appear. When either of two factors, but 
not both, is a sufficient cause of an effect, it must be shown that the effect 
follows when either factor appears, but that it does not follow if neither factor 
appears and if both factors appear. Finally, where either, or both, of two 
factors is a sufficient cause, it must be shown that the effect follows the 
appearance of either factor, or both together, but does not follow if neither 
factor appears. 

Finally, we considered the problems of multifactor analysis. We saw that 
the interpretation of any story involves an analysis of concomitant variation 
of related elements in other stories. We found that any story might be an 
expression of the individual's values, or the threats which beset him, or 
characteristics of the individual which are invariant, or the conditions necessary 
for any clement within the story. 



CHAPTER V 
LEVEL ANALYSIS 

The determination of the relationship between story and storyteller is 
the keystone of interpretation. This relationship can never be less complex 
than the one between phantasy and the larger matrix of the personality. 
Considering the nature of the imaginative productions called for by the TAT, 
it may be even more complex. The individual is asked for something more 
than his fleeting, diffuse, private phantasies. He is called upon to interpret 
the behavior, feelings, and expectations of individuals represented pictorially. 
It is for this reason that the storyteller in his interpretation of the lives of 
these characters may expose fragments of his own past history, his contem- 
porary behavior, and his future expectations and aspirations. He may reveal 
his public behavior, those facets shared only with intimates, and private feelings 
guarded from any public scrutiny as well as wishes whose existence has been 
somehow guarded from even the individual's own awareness. We will address 
ourselves in this chapter to the unraveling of these diverse strands from the 
fabric of TAT protocol. 

A. THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN OVERT AND COVERT NEEDS 

An individual may have wishes which he does not translate into overt 
behavior; this is one of the salient characteristics of any socialized human 
being. There is considerable evidence for the assumption that such wishes 
are frequently attributed to the heroes of the individual's TAT stories. One of 
the most systematic explorations of this area occurs in Sanford's (84) study 
of school children. He examined the relationship between needs as they are 
expressed in the TAT and as they are expressed in overt behavior. The 
average correlation was -\-o.n 9 from which he concluded that we cannot 
assume that a need which appears in TAT stories will be manifested in 
the behavior of that individual. He found evidence that certain needs most 
commonly inhibited by the children appeared most frequently in their TAT's. 
Thus there was a correlation of about 0.30 between TAT expression and 
overt expression of the following needs: acquisition, aggression, autonomy, 
harm avoidance, sex, and succorance. He also found needs which were of low 
intensity in the TAT but high in overt behavior. These needs were: under- 
standing, order, counteraction, blame avoidance, deference, construction and 
sentience. Finally there were needs showing high intensity in both the TAT 
and overt behavior: achievement, nurturance, affiliation, dominance and cog- 
nizance. The explanation offered for the presence in the TAT of antisocial 

55 



56 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

needs which did not appear in overt behavior was that cultural prohibition 
or internal conflict prevented the overt gratification of these needs and thereby 
increased their intensity on the level of phantasy. Needs found to be low in 
the TAT but high in overt behavior pioved to be socially acceptable needs 
whose expression was not only permitted but encouraged and which conse- 
quendy left little residual tension to stimulate phantasy. To explain needs 
which appeared high in both the TAT and overt behavior it was assumed that 
these were encouraged by our culture but given insufficient opportunity for 
satisfaction, either through circumstances or because the individual lacked 
ability to satisfy them. To account further for the similarity between phantasy 
and behavior, two additional hypotheses were suggested. First, that needs in 
conflict with either social or personal ideals would appear both in phantasy 
and behavior if the need were strong and self control weak. Second, that needs 
socially acceptable if expressed in one way and socially unacceptable if other- 
wise expressed might be high in both phantasy and behavior but expressed 
more primitively in phantasy than in behavior. Evidence for the second 
hypothesis was found in a correlation of about +0.30 between the need sex 
as it appeared in the TAT and the needs affiliation, nurturance, and deference 
as these appeared in overt behavior. 

These hypotheses concerning the relationship between needs in phantasy 
and those expressed in overt behavior are not contradicted by any contemporary 
knowledge in the field of personality. Why, then, do the intercorrelations 
offered in support of these hypotheses typically average about +0.30? It is 
our opinion that the dynamic relationships postulated by Sanford are valid 
hypotheses of the relation between covert needs and overt behavior and perhaps 
between phantasy and behavior. But we do not believe that the quantitative 
scoring of needs in TAT protocols provides us with an accurate estimate of 
the strength of either phantasy or covert needs. We have presented evidence 
in Chapter I that at least 30 per cent of the TAT reflects the past history of 
the individual, that in general every level of psychological function finds 
representation within the stories. Repressed and suppressed, covert and overt 
needs, the past, the present and the future all these are woven into the 
imaginative productions of the TAT. For this reason we cannot assume that 
correlations between needs expressed in the TAT and needs expressed in 
behavior represent correlations between covert and overt needs, or even between 
phantasy and behavior. The TAT represents more than the individual's 
private phantasies. 

The relationship between overt and covert needs can frequently be found 
within the TAT stories themselves if attention is given to the levels and 
qualifiers portrayed in the story. Scoring according to either the Murray- 
Sanford needs or our vectors does not differentiate the level of psychological 



LEVEL ANALYSIS 57 

function or specific qualifications of such a function. Both Murray and San- 
ford score one qualifier that of the intensity of the need or press but dif- 
ferences in level or contingency or temporal dimension are not scored. The 
statement, "He achieved what he had worked for," and the statement, "He 
wanted to work and achieve great success," are both rated need achievement, 
and the only qualification of this rating would be the degree of intensity of 
the need. The fact that one represents actual work and achieved success and 
that the other represents these same needs on the level of wish is not indicated 
in the scoring of a need, except insofar as there are differences in intensity. 
In the interest of flexibility and economy of language it is desirable that needs 
or vectors be scored without reference to levels or other qualifications. But 
if we are to understand the relationship between covert needs and overt 
behavior these differences must somewhere be included in the scoring of the 
protocols. The qualifiers of contingency and temporal characteristics are no 
less important for this purpose. It is one thing for the hero to say, "He will 
probably achieve success," but another to say, "Ten years later he did achieve 
success." Although both events are placed into the future, the latter indicates 
a greater degree of confidence. Again, the statement, "He will achieve suc- 
cess," means something other than the statement, "He was successful in the 
past." The first might be the statement of an individual looking hopefully 
into the future, the other the longing for a return to a golden age. Needs 
and press, vectors and conditions, without further specification of level and 
other qualifiers cannot yield important functional relationships either within 
the TAT or between TAT and the personality of the story-teller. The first 
step toward an understanding of the level of psychological function repre- 
sented in TAT protocol is an analysis of the degree of variance of the levels 
employed in each protocol. 

B. DEGREE OF VARIANCE OF LEVEL 

We sometimes find that a protocol may be invariant with respect to the 
level on which the stories proceed. Stress may produce in variance of level 
in an entire protocol. An example is that of the Marine who discovered that 
his wife had been unfaithful to him while he had been overseas. In his TAT 
every one of his heroes "worried and wondered how everything was going 
to turn out". The content of this worry and wondering was not specified 
nor was there interaction between the characters of his stories. This invariance 
of the level of feeling and expectation was a representation of the constriction 
of his functioning and the hypertrophy of these two levels induced by his 
unhappiness. 

If the testing situation itself significantly raises the level of anxiety, a not 
uncommon sequel is invariance of stories on the level of object description: 



58 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

"The violin looks like a Stradivarius," "This woman is middle aged," "This 
is a farm scene and they are farmers and she is a student with a book." These 
are typical of the "stories" such individuals tell, commenting "I don't get much 
out of that." 

Occasionally one finds protocols which employ only the behavioral level. 
"This boy is playing the violin. When he finishes he goes out and plays ball, 
and then he comes and eats supper." "The farmer is plowing the field 
and the woman helps him. The girl is going to school and when she gets 
back she will do the washing." Such individuals, we have found, are extra- 
verted to such an extent that their behavior is imbedded in the external world 
and they are free to a great extent of endopsychic barriers which might inter- 
fere with the spontaneous expression of their wishes. Wish and behavior are 
one and the same for these individuals. They are aware of a wish only as it is 
reflected in their behavior, in the manner in which one who is gardening 
does not differentiate between himself and hoe. Although the hoe is the 
instrument of the person, the individual, if there are no obstacles to his activity, 
experiences only the "hoe activity" as an undifferentiated whole. 

Other individuals may be so deeply engrossed in their inner life that 
instead of assimilating their inner life to the external world they assimilate 
the world to their inner life. If this is the case we may find an invariance of 
the level of feeling or thought or memory throughout the protocols. Com- 
merce with the external world on the behavioral level is evaluated in terms 
of its effects on this inner life. Thus in the following story the violin becomes 
a part of the hero's inner life. 

A youth at dreams. Into many lives come moments which are extremely 
enlightening. Those may be the times of daydreams, or they may be the 
beginnings of a reality. A young lad sat as in a sleep over his best of friends 
in a mechanical way. It was as if his best friend were giving a support for 
his dreams. Those friends are generally the ones which become a part of 
one's own being. As this child sat over his violin in a dream, he saw his 
future in a sort of way. He dreamed of his playing before the last thrones 
of the world; he dreamed that he was the creator of a new world in music. 
From that Saturday morning dream came a great man. Today Dr. X has 
made his life shape around his friend over which he visioned his future. He 
is only one of many men of vision who took time out to dream. 

Or the inner life of feeling may assume proportions which completely 
remove the individual from interaction with the external world. Stories in 
such cases employ the level of feeling or mood throughout as in the following 
example: 

This is the picture of a man who is very depressed. Nothing seems to matter 
any more what's the use. He has the feeling there is nothing inside of him 
no feelings nothing. It was better before when he felt miserable. 



LEVEL ANALYSIS 59 

There are protocols which portray one or two levels exclusively. But 
this is rare. In such cases, as tve have seen, the level which is invariant is a 
literal representation of the -predominant level on which that individual 
functions. 

C. RELATIVE FREQUENCY OF LEVELS 

Most individuals, however, operate on many levels. They behave. Events 
happen. They perceive, attend to, and take interest in the world about them. 
They have plans. They are governed in part by expectations and by their 
sentiments and wishes. They have feelings and physical sensations. They 
remember and they have daydreams and night dreams. But there are impor- 
tant differences in the relative time and energy devoted by different individuals 
to these levels. A very rough index of these differences may be achieved from 
the relative frequency of appearance of each level in the protocol. This type 
of analysis is particularly valuable in the comparison of cultural differences. 
For example the TAT stories given by Navaho children are characterized 
by a predominance of behavior and event, with relatively little elaboration of 
personal feelings, memories, daydreams, and so on. Further evidence of the 
usefulness of such a technique of analysis may be found in a consideration 
of the data presented in Kutash's (49) study of the TATs of psychopathic 
defective criminals. These individuals, suffering restraint of behavior because 
of their imprisonment, might be expected to employ the levels of object descrip- 
tion, feeling, and wish more frequently than the level of behavior in their 
stories. Examination of his findings confirms this expectation. The category 
of highest frequency was separation anxiety, the next most frequent category 
was description, and third was the category ambition a wish to achieve some- 
thing in the future. The responses next in frequency were conflicts and guilt 
feelings. These results are also illuminating in connection with the discrepancy 
between TAT findings and overt behavior. Stories involving the wish to 
achieve great things and thereby to make people happy are indeed discrepant 
with the behavior but not with the total personality of these individuals. 
They do wish to achieve much and to redeem themselves in the eyes of society. 
These wishes are expressed in their stories. There is, however, no less 
discrepancy between their wishes and their actual behavior than between the 
TAT and their behavior. 

D. CAUSE-EFFECT RELATIONS OF LEVELS 

Although a comparison of the relative frequency of a variety of levels 
may yield a general picture of the level economy of the individual, much more 
specific analysis is required to evaluate the meaning of different levels as they 
are attributed to the heroes of the TAT stories. The level attributed to the 
hero may be characteristic of the personality of his creator if it is invariant 



60 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

throughout the stories. But since this is relatively infrequent we must employ 
another technique. A level may vary from story to story for two quite different 
reasons: It may be either the cause or the effect of some condition which varies. 
Let us consider the following two stories told by a young man. 

This looks like a fellow looking out of the window, just dreaming. He's 
dreaming of the day when he'll be head man at his office. They're beautiful 
thoughts and ten years later we find him at the same window dreaming the 
same beautiful thoughts. He's still a clerk. 

He's a tired, but very happy man. Today he realized the goal of years 
of hard work. It wasn't easy, slaving hours when all the others had gone 
home at five o'clock, but it was worth it. 

We are told in effect that daydreaming results in failure, and hard work 
leads to success. This was the protocol of an extremely industrious young man 
whose actual behavior is illustrated by the second story. The first story repre- 
sents his ideology and his belief that if he were to relax his efforts and give 
himself over to daydreaming he would certainly fail. Differences in level 
here are causes or conditions; the important variable is the success which 
he feels can be accomplished only through work on the behavioral level. The 
hero who daydreams is not a projection of either the behavior or the wish 
of the storyteller, but a reflection of a belief about the consequences of the 
level of daydreaming. It is for this reason that an analysis of the relative fre- 
quency of various levels must be interpreted with caution. It may by no means 
indicate that the individual daydreams as much as he works if, as in this 
protocol, each appears with the same frequency. 

Let us now consider the case in which a variation in level appears as the 
effect of preceding conditions. The following two stories are illustrative. 

He's a very hard-working young man. He's brought some work home from 
the office to do. He's interested in it and in no time at all he finishes it and 
goes to bed. 

He's got some kind of a problem probably ran up against something that was 
too much for him. Let's say he was working on something and he ran up 
against a tough problem. Finally he loses interest in it and begins to think 
of other things. And let's round it off by his daydreaming of being the 
smartest man in his line of work. 

We are told that when the hero is interested in his work and there are 
no serious barriers to the solution of his problem he is a hard worker and 
finishes his job. But when insurmountable difficulties arise he takes refuge 
in daydreams of glory. In this case the variations of level do not tell us that 
he is half worker and half day dreamer. It is rather a projection of the 
conditions which allow the individual to work, or which inhibit work and 



LEVEL ANALYSIS 6l 

force him to the level of daydream. Whether in actual fact this individual 
works hard or daydreams cannot be determined by a comparison of these 
stories. We can predict the conditions under which he will and will not be 
able to carry his work through to a successful completion but we would have 
to inquire further, either in the TAT or other sources, to determine the rela- 
tive frequency of these levels of function in the life of the individual. 

The analysis of the levels of psychological function should employ the 
techniques oudined in the preceding chapter. The meaning of the appearance 
of any level in a story must be evaluated in terms of the concomitant variation 
of other elements in the stories. As we have seen, its introduction may signify 
that the particular level is a resultant of specific conditions, and that when 
these conditions are different, the resultant level changes. Or the specific level 
may signify that it is the specific condition of a certain sequel, and that if 
the hero's level of function were to change, the sequelae would be different. 

E. SEQUENCE ANALYSIS OF LEVELS 

Having determined the relative frequency of levels and the causal rela- 
tions between levels and other dimensions of the stories, we may now turn 
to another technique of analysis. This consists in a systematic examination 
of the sequence of levels, 

I. SEQUELAE OF THE LEVEL OF WISH 

Let us consider first the sequence of levels which may follow the expres- 
sion of a wish. The wish may be followed by a change to the level of thinking 
and then by the level of behavior. Generally such sequences signify a delibera- 
tive conjunctive personality for whom the expression of any wish necessarily 
involves an intermediate step in thinking, as in the following story. 

Well, it looks as though the violin is broken and he feels sad about it because 
he likes to play. Uh . . . he's about eight years old and he wants to be a 
violinist. His family can't afford to get him another one. He's probably 
thinking about how he can earn some money to get it repaired. He gets a 
job delivering newspapers and gets the violin repaired and takes up his 
studies again. Because of his interest he becomes a success. 

The wish may be followed by thinking but without further translation 
into behavior. This sequence signifies an overemphasis on the deliberative 
phase of activity, so that action is paralyzed by excessive planning. 

This little boy is an only child. He has great ambitions, but he knows it 
won't be easy. He'll have to learn how to read music and then he'll have to 
learn to play scales, and then he'll have to work very hard and practice 
10 hours a day, but he thinks he can do it and he figures out how many 
years it will take him to accomplish all of this, and we see him here 
figuring it all out. 



62 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

A similar sequence is that of wish-thinking-daydream. This sequence 
differs from the preceding one in that the overelaboration of the level of 
thinking does not paralyze action, but rather short-circuits the wish through 
the level of daydream. 

The little boy wants to be a violinist and he goes about it very methodically. 
He figures on asking his parents if he can take violin lessons, and if they 
say yes he plans on getting the best teacher they can afford, and as he thinks 
of all of this he sees himself on the stage at Carnegie Hall with thousands of 
people spellbound at the greatest violinist of the age. 

In another common sequence, the wish is followed by daydreaming and 
this followed by behavior. In these cases the means-end cognizance is less 
important to the individual than the vision of the future goal which inspires 
him to translate the wish into appropriate behavior. This sequence of levels 
is represented in the following story. 

The girl is daydreaming. She wants to become a great artist, the kind that 
people admire. She is a very smart girl and she wants to make a name or 
reputation for herself so she studies diligently and develops into a renowned 
person. 

But the sequence wish-daydream may also be followed by an expectation 
of an event rather than by behavior. In the following story the boy wishes 
he could play the violin; this leads to a daydream which leads to the expecta- 
tion that he will someday be a violinist; the latter is an event and the object 
of the expectation. Daydreams in this case create sufficient certainty to require 
no behavior on the part of the hero. 

This boy wishes more than anything that he might be able to play the violin 
like his father. He dreams of the day when he'll be old enough. He's certain 
that when he does get older he will be a great violinist like his father. 

The wish may however lead to a daydream which becomes an end in 
itself, preceded by no thinking and with neither behavior nor the expectation 
of behavior or event following the daydream. There is in such a case even 
less value to the daydream as an instrument for evoking behavior. In the 
preceding two stories the daydreams served the function of either inspiring 
work, or at the least, creating confidence in a future event, but here it merely 
has the effect of short-circuiting the wish. The following is an example of 
such a sequence. 

This kid is daydreaming about being a great violinist. He's always wanted 
to be a violinist, and here he sits staring at the violin imagining how wonderful 
it would be. He's so lost in reverie and completely absorbed that he forgets 
about everything else. 



LEVEL ANALYSIS 63 

Another variety of passivity following a wish is the sequence wish-day- 
dream-event, in which the event is the result of the behavior of another person. 
This represents either the story of an individual who has in fact received an 
abundance of gratuities without effort on his part or a wish fulfillment phan- 
tasy. The following story was told by an individual who had received many 
gratuities in his childhood: 

The little boy has been wishing he might have a violin. Wouldn't it be 
wonderful, he thought, to have my own violin like father has. One day as he 
was dreaming about this his father marches in and says look what I've got 
for you, and sure enough it's a violin. 

The wish may be followed by an event without elaboration on the level 
of daydream. The event may gratify or frustrate the satisfaction of the wish, 
but, in either case, the individual is pictured as the passive object of external 
forces. The following two stories provide examples of this sequence. 

This young kid wishes he could be violinist. But his parents don't have much 
money and that's that. He never becomes one. 

The young boy pictured here is staring enraptured at the violin he's just 
gotten. He's wanted a violin for a long time and had about given up hope, 
but his parents at last bought him one. 

The wish followed immediately by behavior is a curiously rare sequence. 
This is probably because the extraverted individual who is imbedded in the 
external world is less aware of the existence of wishes as separate entities. The 
following story is representative of this sequence. 

This boy is tired of playing the violin, he'd rather be playing ball anyway, so 
he puts down the violin and goes out and plays ball with the kid next door. 

The wish may be followed by neither daydream, intention, nor behavior, 
but stand alone, ungratified. Such a story is not uncommonly told by an 
individual whose wishes, by their very nature, seem impossible of gratification. 

This is a very peculiar child. He has always wanted to play his mother's 
violin. Otherwise he's a perfectly normal child. We see him here looking at 
his mother's violin, wishing he could play it. 

The wish may be followed by a counter wish and then behavior. The 
behavior may be directed toward fulfilling either the former or latter wish. If 
the first wish leads to an inhibiting wish, and the behavior expresses the 
inhibiting wish, this signifies an individual who yields to the forces of inhibi- 
tion-. But if the behavior represents the original wish, it is an indication of 
an impulsive personality, but somewhat less impulsive than if the counter wish 
had not been mentioned. The following two stories are representative of these 
sequences. 



64 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

Johnny wants to get out and play still he should practice. Doesn't he want 
to be a great violinist? After a while he will pick up the violin and practice. 

This chap is in a fix. Today is the first day of spring, and there's a swell 
game going on just outside his house. It isn't that he doesn't like to practice 
the violin, he does, but he likes to play baseball too, especially today. He 
plays baseball and has a wonderful time. He forgets all about his violin till 
he hears his mother calling him into the house for supper, and then he 
remembers, and he feels a little bad because the next day his music teacher is 
coming and he hasn't yet memorized the piece he was supposed to. 

2. INSTIGATORS OF THE LEVEL OF WISH 

We have thus far considered common sequences of levels which are 
sequelae to the level of wish. We will now consider the levels which precede 
and instigate the level of wish. 

A wish may be instigated or reactivated by events. Protocols characterized 
by such relationships signify an individual whose inner life is essentially 
reactive to outer stimuli of an impersonal nature. In the following story the 
rumor of an event reactivates a wish the hero has always possessed. 

Ah here's our circus performer a very agile tumbler, a gymnast. He's climb- 
ing up so he can look out, hmm, he sees something I can't figure. It's at a 
circus, though that straight line back there doesn't look like a circus. And it's 
while they're setting up the show. Oh, yes. He's heard the rumor that the 
elephants have broken loose and he always wanted to see what would happen 
if the elephants broke loose* He had been a gym teacher up to a few months 
ago, before he joined the circus. And he gets a big kick out of this. And 
now he's running up the rope to see if the elephants are really loose. And 
sure enough they are. There's a couple running downtown not fast or doing 
any damage just loping slowly, ambling along. The women faint and the 
men cheer (laughs) and it's a big excitement in this middle sized town till 
the trainer comes along. 

Wishes may also be activated or reactivated by memory. Individuals who 
suffer grief, shame, or anxiety may tell such stories. One of the salient 
characteristics of such inner states is their peculiar sensitivity to rearousal 
through memory. In our experience the sequence memory-wish has usually 
signified serious disturbance of the inner life of the individual telling the 
story. In the following story the hero's wish for the days of slavery is 
reactivated by the memory of his ex-master. 

This picture takes place in the i88o's. Shown here is an ex-slave praying at 
the tomb of his master. Every year this man performs this act on the 
anniversary of the night his ex-master was killed during the fight following 
the Civil War. This Negro longs again for the days of slavery although all 
his younger friends laugh at him. 



LEVEL ANALYSIS 65 

The wish may be activated by physical sensations. This is particularly 
characteristic of children and adolescents, who are apt to be governed to a 
great extent by their bodily states. In the following story told by an adolescent 
girl, the combination of fatigue and hunger activates the wish to return home. 

Somebody's going to have a child, I see. Or is that just the picture? Well, 
this girl's family was illiterate and she was quite a bright person and wanted 
to go to school. So when they moved over here from Jugoslavia they look 
like they came from Jugoslavia she immediately registered in school. How- 
ever, with the Mother nagging that she should stay at home, she didn't do as 
well as she should. She could have done better. Finally; the nagging got so 
bad she decided to leave. Oh, she got about fifty or sixty miles from the 
house, realized she was very tired and hungry, started to wish she hadn't left. 
Meantime her parents were frantic. Both were feeling sorry for the other 
one parents for the child and the child for the parents and both decided 
to give in to each other. Now, how to finish? After a week or so, kid 
starving, the child returned home due to a policeman's kindness and her 
excellent memory. She knew how to get home but was scared. She finished 
up school and married a rising lawyer. Moral: Go to school and don't let 
your parents interfere with you. 

A common activator of any wish is an expectation. This may be the 
individual's own expectation, that of another person, or both. In the following 
story, the wish to make a success of playing the violin is intensified by the 
hero's thought of how much his mother counted on his success, and the 
expectation of his mother's disappointment should he fail. In this case 
the content of his own expectation is* his mother's wish. 

Why must I practice so hard? and so long? and every day? And yet I love 
Mother so. I must make a success of this violin playing. Oh I must! Mother 
is counting on it; it's everything to her; if I fail and can't become very 
famous as ... well the Great Jonny Jerome Mrs. Albert Jerome's Jonny Jerome 
. . . that would be better still, I certainly will deserve the booby prize. Then 
Mother can dress up and feel very proud and forget her failure. Mother, 
gosh, she must have been very pretty. Somehow they didn't see how talented 
she was ... or maybe her voice was too small ... it sounds so pretty here 
in the apartment but maybe it didn't sound like that in the big opera 
houses; maybe that's why she wasn't a success. I've got to work hard, real 
hard, terribly hard so she will be pleased. Tommy and Bud just don't under- 
stand why I'm so gone on this fiddle and catgut. It's Mother when some 
extra special sound comes out she is so happy. Gee, I want an awful lot of 
extra special sounds to come 'cause she seems to even have Dad with her 
in this room here on earth, not really but just pretend . . . This A string 
doesn't sound quite true there she is ... turn . . . turn ... tee ... turn, 
there it was right that time, but maybe I'd better do that scale once more; 
then I can go on to that new one. Gosh, I'm getting sleepy. Now, Jonny 
Jerome, are you going to sleep when you have another hour to practice? Here 



66 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

goes turn ... tee ... turn. This is the last time for the scale D E F G A, 
then I can go on to the new one . . . turn ... tee ... turn. T can't ... go 
... to ... t* ... sleep . . . slee . . . slee. The following is an excerpt from 
a local newspaper printed some 10 years later: "A young, blond, and hand- 
some chap, known to this town's other young and handsome chaps as 'Jonny* 
and to the 'olsters' as 'Mrs Albert Jerome's son who plays the violin so weir 
has been claimed by both 'chaps' and 'olsters' as the GREAT JEROME. Last 
evening he strummed and plucked those strings as no other seventeen year old 
since Paganini has ever dared. If we are not mistaken we see a brilliant 
future for this violinist. Mrs. Albert Jerome may well be proud of such a 



Not uncommon is the activation of a wish by a special state, such as 
intoxication. The presence of a special state as instigator generally signifies 
that the wish is under some inhibition in the normal state, and some de-inhibit- 
ing force is required to activate it. 

These two are lovers. They have known each other for a long time. Up to 
this point they have had a very platonic relationship, but tonight they are both 
feeling pretty high after a couple of highballs, and they are both suddenly 
seized with a passion neither of them had ever felt before. We see them here 
afterwards. She is naked, and he is sobering up and wondering what ever 
came over him. They will both feel upset for some time to come, and it will 
be a long time before they see each other again. 

Wishes may be activated through the perception of human objects in 
the external world. This generally signifies an individual whose inner life 
is governed in large part by the presence of other human beings. In the 
following story the perception of a beautiful woman activates the wish for 
intimacy. 

This is a man standing under a street light, just hanging around. As he stands 
there the most beautiful woman he thought he had ever seen in his life 
walked by. He felt himself suddenly moved deeply he must see her, talk to 
her, touch her but how? He followed her till she went into an apartment 
building and he hesitated. He could not follow her. But he could and did 
wait evening after evening outside her apartment to catch a glimpse of her, 
and eventually he talked to her and then they became more and more intimate 
till one day he asked her to marry him. They were married and it was only 
then that she told him that she had felt exactly the same way that he did that 
first night, and had hoped he would follow her. 

Wishes may be activated by the behavior of others, rather than by their 
presence. This sequence is typical of individuals whose wishes are essentially 
reactive to the behavior of others. In the following story the individual's sex 
wishes are incited by the seductive behavior of a woman: 

The woman in bed is naked. The man was a boarder in the same house. 
He had never been interested in women, but she was always leaving her door 



LEVEL ANALYSIS 6j 

open, and exposing herself half naked to him. Tonight she was more 
forward, and came into his room in negligee. As she touched him he 
suddenly felt he wanted her and they slept together. Now he is full of 
remorse. 

Finally, wishes may be activated through the medium of moods or feelings. 
This may signify an individual whose inner life exerts massive pressure and 
is more self regulative than reactive to outer stimuli. In the following story 
the mood of bitterness and despair activates the wish for self destruction. 

This man is at the end of his rope. His life was full of bitterness and 
black despair. He curses the destiny that brought him to such degradation. 
He broods and broods, till one day he feels he would rather die than live 
on like this. But though he wants to be free, he cannot bring himself to 
take his own life. 

We have thus far considered single instigators of wishes. Usually, a 
sequence of levels responsible for the evocation of a wish involves complex 
chains of determinants. Let us examine one such example. In the following 
story the initial level is behavior the young man leaves home the level then 
changes to event he is surrounded by the darkness of the night. This, and 
the light coming from homes, leads him to the level of thinking he considers 
these happy homes. The level of thinking changes to the level of mood 
he is lonely and depressed and this mood leads to the wish to be back home 
with his family. Thus the sequence is behavior-evcnt-thinking-mood-wish. 

This young man has left home in an effort to make his way in another 
part of the world. At the point of this picture he's standing outside, sur- 
rounded by the darkness of the night which is spotted here and there with 
the warm light of what he considers happy homes. He is lonely and some- 
what depressed and wishes that he were back home now with his family. In 
the near future this feeling will not be as frequent and when it does come 
will not burden him to the. extreme it does this first time he is feeling it 
He will become a man of die world with little regard for sentiment in other 
people although always very sentimental himself. 

In the preceding examples we have considered the sequence of levels 
preceding and following a particular level, that of wish. The same type of 
analysis may be applied to any other level. We may analyze the sequence 
of levels which precede and follow events, behavior, intentions, thoughts, 
moods, and feelings. Documentation of each of these varieties of analysis 
would require encyclopedic treatment beyond the scope of this book, but the 
logic of this type of analysis is clear. We examine the protocol with respect 
to sequence of levels, disregarding the content of the level. By means of this 
type of analysis patterns of such level-sequences may be extracted which might 
otherwise escape attention because they may be embedded in a variety of 



68 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

heterogeneous strivings and conditions. Interpretation of these sequences is 
governed by the logic of all interpretation. We cannot assume that a sequence 
literally represents something typical of the storyteller unless that sequence is 
invariant. If it is not invariant we must examine the conditions under which 
the sequences may vary to determine the meaning of the variability. Thus 
we might examine the effect of any single level on the sequence of levels 
which follows. Let us suppose that the typical level sequence of a conjunctive 
deliberate individual is that of wish-intention-thinking-behavior, in which the 
hero implements his wish by planning to do something, then considers the 
various steps to be taken, and finally does it. We might then examine the 
influence of any other level preceding the wish, to determine whether any 
particular level changes this typical sequence. We might discover that if the 
level of wish is preceded by mood or feeling, let us say depression or anxiety, 
the sequence changes to a less deliberative one in which the wish leads immedi- 
ately to impulsive behavior, or to a more deliberate sequence in which the 
wish leads to excessive planning and conflicting expectations which paralyze 
behavior. 

F. ANALYSIS OF AWARENESS 

We have thus far addressed ourselves to the problem of the relationship 
between the various levels of psychological function as they are reflected in 
the TAT. Another problem is the degree to which the individual is aware of 
his own wishes or behavior. It is not uncommon for individuals to be unaware 
of the determinants of their feelings. An individual may be anxious or 
depressed and be unaware either of why he feels so, or even what the object 
of the anxiety or depression may be. Less frequently an individual may be 
anxious or depressed and not "know" it, then suddenly realize that he has had 
these feelings during the day and been only dimly aware of them. The extent 
of awareness of behavior may be similarly limited. Such awareness of the 
nature of overt behavior may be limited to an awareness of single concrete 
fragments of behavior. The patterning of behavioral sequences may entirely 
escape the attention of the individual. The contemporary determinants of such 
patterns are even more elusive, and the relationship of these to their genetic 
precursors is the achievement of only the most reflective individuals. 

Determination of the degree and scope of awareness o the storyteller, as 
this is revealed in his TAT stories, is a relatively simple procedure. In our 
experience the storyteller cannot attribute such awareness to his heroes unless 
he possesses an equivalent degree of awareness of his own wishes and behavior. 
When stories are told without causal or motivational connections woven into 
the fabric of the story, this typically reflects a similar lack of awareness in the 
personality of the storyteller. 



LEVEL ANALYSIS 69 

Consider the individual who tells a story of a hero who has been devoted 
to his mother all his life and who because of this regrets very much that he 
must leave home. The hero finds no happiness in his work away from home 
because he misses his mother, and consequently becomes more and more 
depressed, finally deciding that the only remedy is to return home. We may 
be certain that the individual telling such a story possesses an equivalent degree 
of awareness of the genetic determinants of his own depression and inability 
to work. 

Suppose, however, that the story told of a hero who feels lonely and 
depressed, and because of this can find no happiness in his work, becomes more 
and more depressed about it and decides, as a result, to go home. Such an 
individual is unlikely to be aware of the genetic determinants of his loneliness, 
and consequently is incapable of attributing them to his hero. 

Let us suppose, on the other hand, that the story told of a hero who finds 
no happiness in his work and decides as a result to return home. In this case 
the individual is unaware that his unhappiness in work may be the result of 
loneliness, even though he returns home because he does not enjoy his work. 

Or the story might have told of a hero who is vaguely unhappy and who 
returns home, with no motivation attributed either to the unhappiness or the 
return home. In this series of examples, such an individual would have least 
awareness of the context of his own motives. 

There are many fine gradations possible in the extent and scope of aware- 
ness. Thus, in the first story, less awareness of the functional relationships 
might have resulted in a story about a hero who has been devoted to his 
mother all his life and who leaves home with regrets and who finds no happi- 
ness in his work and who becomes more and more depressed and finally 
returns home. All the motivational connections in this story have been omitted; 
instead, there is a series of only partially related events and feelings. Such 
a story would be told by an individual less aware of his own motivation and 
the larger matrix in which it is embedded than the individual capable of telling 
the first story. 

One typically finds in the TAT stories of patients who have received 
psychotherapy attribution to their heroes of complex patterns of motivation. 
Psychotherapy is of course not the only road to such insight. In the following 
series of stories, we present "insightful" stories from 3 individuals who have 
been psychoanalyzed, and 3 whose stories were told without benefit of prior 
therapeutic intervention. 

In this story, told by a young woman who had renounced her tomboy 
wishes with regret, we are given insight into the dynamics of this change. 

She's sitting in a room looking out of a window and she's watching he*r 
brothers and their friends outside playing baseball It's spring. She's very 



yo THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

proud of her brothers but she's also wishing she were a boy and could play 
with them too. She's about fifteen years old. What happens to her is, 
she reverts from her tomboy wishes and aspirations and becomes extremely 
feminine in a couple of years. She sort of realizes she can't be a boy, 
subconsciously, and she decides that, if she can't, at least she wants them to 
notice her some way or other, and she does it by becoming very popular 
so that all her brothers' friends take her out. 

In the following story by an adult, we are told of the motives underlying 
his adolescent interest in boxing lessons. Again, this represents a page from 
the past history of the individual. 

Here is a picture of a young man participating in a gym class. The young 
man was probably underdeveloped as a young child and consequently was 
subject for the taunts of his fellow youths for his inability to play or 
participate in the more strenuous games. With a feeling of inferiority he 
made up his mind that he would take every means at his disposal to develop 
himself. Entering into gym classes he worked hard and in time did develop 
the bulging muscles and the stamina that seemed so necessary to win the 
respect of his fellow youths. Here in this picture he is winning the rope- 
climbing event and, from the expression on his face, a great deal of 
satisfaction. 

In the next story told by a middle-aged woman there is realization, at one 
point, that the material is autobiographical, but despite this the logic of her 
motivation is pursued to the end. 

Oh she is a nasty little bitch really. She's made him so angry he's trying to 
get away. She is turning on everything she has got to keep him. She hates 
to lose control of the situation. She has been spoiled all her life. She can't 
stand to be rejected by anyone. This sounds autobiographical. She can be 
successfully sweet and charming her charm is disarming that is an awkward 
way to put it. She is the kind most people can't know well. She's got such a 
skillful technique. . That kind of person is dangerous the greater the 
charm, the greater the viciousness underneath, 

In the story which follows the individual correctly diagnoses the relation- 
ship between his paranoid ideas and parental indoctrination: 

This chap is a paranoid. Feels wherever he goes someone's about to follow, 
choke or seize him. Can't get the idea out of his mind. Unable to sleep 
at night because he thinks someone is under the bed. Wherever he goes 
he has this terrible sensation of being followed. He finally ends up in an 
institution where his obsession is located but had progressed so far that it 
is impossible to eliminate it He ends up hopelessly nuts. His mother was 
oversolicitous of his behavior. As a youngster she told him when he 
walked on the street that wherever he went there were eyes at every window 
which were criticizing his behavior. Therefore he must always behave right 
or he would bring dishonor to his family. You can say that again too. The 



LEVEL ANALYSIS Jl 

last is a personal experience. I used to be afraid to move out of the house 
when I was seven or eight because of the eyes that were supposed to be 
watching. 

The young woman who told the next story describes accurately her tech- 
nique ot coping with her feelings of aggression. 

That hideous woman at die back is this woman's evil nature which she has 
long suppressed. This woman wasn't really evil when a child but things went 
against her. She felt frustrated on every hand. Now however she wants to 
restore her own mental health by attaching herself to some great and selfless 
cause. The more she strives in this direction the fainter and fainter grows 
the woman at the back. Eventually the finer qualities in her are completely 
reinstated. 

And finally the self diagnosis in the case of Joseph Kidd, reported by 
White (104), as this was revealed in one o his TAT stories. 

The story of a youth who throughout life considered himself persecuted and 
unhappy because of others' attitude toward him. He never quite under- 
stood why people did not like him, taking any attitude people had towards 
him as being one of hate. And thus finally he turned away from outside 
activities altogether, remaining aloof from people whom he had come to fear, 
having no friends, living only by himself. He developed an attitude of 
animosity toward his fellow men and wanted to become a greater success so 
that he could in this way dominate and as a result satisfy his persecutory 
complexes. He never realized that perhaps the fault lay in himself. The 
people's attitude toward him depended on himself, not on other people, but 
for years he struggled under the impression that the trouble was on the 
outside, not that anything he would do was wrong; until the day he came 
to the realization, having met a girl he loved, that the world was right and 
he was wrong, that all his trouble, unhappiness, general lethargy was a 
result of his own imagination, and in an attempt to get on a normal footing 
with the outside world for the sake of this girl, he found it was too late- 
the little part of his life had come and gone, and that to start out again 
he would have to revert to a second childhood, and could in no way adjust 
his feelings of animosity to the outside world, with the final result being that 
it only led to greater introversion. The more he tried the more secluded 
became his life until he desperately committed suicide. 

Each of these stories represents incisive insights into the nature of the 
dynamics of the motivation of the individual who told the story. Such attribu- 
tion to these heroes reflects the genuine awareness these individuals possessed 
of the dynamics of their own behavior. It is possible for an individual to 
possess such knowledge and not attribute it to his heroes, but we have not 
found individuals who attribute more awareness of motivation to their heroes 
than the awareness they themselves possess of their own motivation. 



J2 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

G. THE PROBLEM OF REPRESSION 

The attribution of complex motivation to the hero of a TAT story may 
indicate an equivalent degree of awareness on the part of the individual who 
tells the story. But there are certainly motives important in the determination 
of behavior which escape the scrutiny of the most self-conscious individual. 
Exploration of the deeper recesses of the personality calls for a more indirect 
technique of analysis. It is a relatively simple matter, if one possesses auto- 
biographical and observational data, to determine whether the material of 
the protocol represents private wishes or overt behavior. Discrepancies between 
what is known of the individual's daily behavior and the behavior of his 
heroes provides a simple criterion for distinguishing whether the latter repre- 
sents overt behavior or covert wish or expectation. We have not elaborated 
indirect techniques useful in this connection, since knowledge of the indi- 
vidual's actual behavior provides the most certain basis for this differentiation. 

The problem of assessing levels of psychological function within the covert 
sphere is more complex and difficult. Although forces within the covert sphere 
may exert varying degrees of pressure for admittance into consciousness or 
translation into behavior, behavioral criteria for the measurement of such 
differences in pressure have not yet been clearly defined. 

The TAT, because of its sensitivity in eliciting repressed material, offers 
a unique opportunity for the investigation of the mechanism of repression. In 
order to exploit these potentialities fully an excursion into the theory of repres- 
sion will be necessary. Let us turn our attention first to Freud's treatment of 
repression. 

I. HISTORY OF THE CONCEPT 

Freud's theory of repression was based upon the empirical datum of 
resistance the temporary blocking of free association. From this phenomenon 
he inferred that there was a force responsible for this inhibition of the asso- 
ciational process. He found further evidence in such phenomena as slips 
of the tongue that the wish might be of sufficient strength to break through 
the repressing force. He further assumed that in neurosis the repressed wish 
exerted pressure on the door of consciousness sufficient to require continual 
vigilance lest a break-through be effected. This much was the insight of 
genius. Whether this mechanism operates universally and, if it does, whether 
the repressed always "returns" or threatens to return, are questions which 
have yet to receive a definitive answer. We have not learned much more 
concerning the nature of this mechanism than Freud originally taught us. 

In order to employ the TAT in the diagnosis o the degree of repression 
it will be necessary to reformulate and extend Freud's theory of repression. 



LEVEL ANALYSIS 73 

2. A THEORY OF REPRESSION 

We shall treat the phenomenon of repression as a special case of the larger 
problem of the determination of the resultant of psychological forces. Let us 
consider first the nature of psychological forces and their relation to personality. 

a) Intensity, Extensity, and Pressure 

We shall assume first that any personality has a finite quantum of energy 
at its disposal. Our second assumption will be the scientific fiction that a per- 
sonality or psychological system may be treated for analytical purposes as a 
closed system. This latter is an assumption which many sciences have found 
useful despite the fact that no single system can really be a "closed" system. 
Physical systems, for example, are certainly "open" to intervention by human 
and social systems, but the exclusion of the influence of the latter upon physical 
systems has simplified the problems of physics and allowed prediction within 
the science of physics. 

If we make both of these assumptions, our next requirement is a unit 
of energy for this closed system. We shall define psychological energy in 
terms of units of psychological pressure and further asssume that any single 
wish within the personality may have any pressure, not exceeding the total 
amount of pressure available to the personality. We shall also assume that 
because of the finite quantum of pressure units available to the person, the 
expenditure of pressure units in one area limits the amount of pressure avail- 
able for other purposes. Thus, if we assume arbitrarily that an individual 
has 400 units of such pressure available for general expenditure, then if one 
wish consumes 200 units of pressure, the remainder of his activity would be 
limited to 200 units of pressure. This might be spread over ten other areas, 
in which case each area would have a pressure of 20 units, or it might be 
expended in one other area which would then have a pressure of 200 units 
or in two other areas, one of which consumed 150 units, the other 50 units 
and so on. 

We shall now assume further that a unit of pressure is the product of 
two components intensity and extensity. Thus 10 units of pressure might be 
the product of a wish with an intensity of 2 units and an extensity of 5 units, 
or conversely, or the product of a wish with an intensity of 10 units and an 
extensity of i unit or conversely. By intensity we mean the strength of the 
wish or drive; by extensity we mean its scope or mass. Consider the fol- 
lowing example. Individual A wants very much to buy a book. Individual B 
has an equally intense wish to go to college. Neither A nor B can afford to 
satisfy his wish. Although the intensity of both wishes may be high, the total 
pressure of B's wish is much higher because of its higher extensity, since 
pressure is defined as the product of intensity times extensity. There would 



74 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

be differential sequelae in the case of A's and B's frustration. B would suffer 
more frustration because of the greater pressure of his wish. Consider further 
a psychophysiological example. A, due to a vitamin A deficiency, has a specific 
craving of high intensity for carrots. B, due to a general malnutrition, has 
a craving of high intensity for any food. Although each individual's wish 
is of equal intensity, the total pressure of B's wish is greater due to the 
greater extensity of his need both psychologically and physiologically. 

The pressure of a wish might be equivalent if it were either of high intensity 
and low extensity, or low intensity and high extensity. For example, the 
pressure of the specific craving of high intensity for carrots might be equivalent 
in pressure to a wish to study, of low intensity and high extensity, on the 
part of a student. 

b) The Relation of Pressure of Conflict to Total Pressure 

Let us now consider the problem of conflict in the light of these distinc- 
tions. It follows from our assumptions that as the total pressure of two wishes 
in conflict increases, the pressure available for the remainder of the system 
decreases. If two wishes which were in conflict consumed, together, 10 units 
of pressure in a 400 unit system, the secondary consequences of this conflict 
for the personality as a whole would not be serious. But as the conflict involved 
more and more of the total pressure available to the personality, the conflict 
would become more and more pathogenic. Thus a conflict between viewing 
one or another moving picture on a particular evening rarely involves patho- 
genic sequelae although the intensity of the wishes may be high, their 
extensity is typically so low that the total pressure of each wish is low, and 
the combined pressure of both wishes is relatively low. But a conflict between 
two wishes of high pressure, each wish involving half of the total pressure 
of the personality, represents civil war within the individual. Similar phe- 
nomena may be seen on the somatic level. The focal infection represented 
in a pimple which has been effectively segregated from interaction with the rest 
of the body ordinarily permits effective functioning of the organism, but a 
systemic infection may mobilize the entire energies of the organism in self 
defense and in so doing leave very little energy for any other purpose the 
individual is "sick." 

c) Relation of the Ratio of Conflicting Pressures to Total Pressure 

Whether a conflict will be pathogenic and, more specifically, whether the 
repressed will "return" is also a function of the relative pressure of the two 
forces which stand in opposition to each other. It will be our hypothesis 
that as the repressing force increases in pressure, relative to the repressed force, 
the possibility of a return of the repressed wish decreases and, further, that this 



LEVEL ANALYSIS 75 

entails a reduction in disturbance to the personality as a whole. Conversely 
we will assume that as the balance between two forces in opposition approaches 
equality there will be a greater tendency of the repressed wish to return and 
an increase in general disturbance to the personality as a whole. Our doctrine 
is, in this respect, contrary to that contemporary opinion which attributes 
serious pathogenic potentialities to "deeply" repressed material. We are main- 
taining in effect that the "deeper" the repression the less pathogenic the con- 
flict. We prefer to speak of the relative pressure of two forces rather than the 
"depth" of repression, since the former is more susceptible of measurement 
than the concept of depth. Let us consider next the relation between this ratio 
and the ratio of the whole conflict to the total pressure. 

d) Relation of Ratio of Conflicting Pressures to the Ratio of Combined 
Pressures of the Conflict to Total Pressure 

We have maintained that a conflict may be pathogenic for one of two 
reasons: either it consumes the entire energies of the system or the opposed 
pressures are evenly balanced. We must now consider the relationship between 
these two factors. 

If our hypotheses are correct, the most serious conflict would be one in 
which there were two fairly evenly balanced^ forces, consuming between them 
the entire energies of the system. The least serious conflict would be that 
between two forces of low pressure, in which one of the forces was relatively 
much higher in pressure than the other, although only a small part of the 
total pressure of the system was expended by the two of them. As a conflict 
approached either of these end points, it would be either more or less patho- 
genic. It would also follow that if one ratio was favorable, the pathogenic 
potentiality of the other ratio would be reduced. Thus, if the conflict were 
evenly balanced between two forces, it would be less pathogenic as the total 
pressure of the conflict decreased. Or if there were a high total pressure 
of the two forces in conflict, it would be less pathogenic as the disparity in 
relative pressure between the two forces increased. 

Let us examine these hypotheses more closely. It would be generally 
admitted that a conflict of two forces of relatively low pressure in which one 
force was of much greater pressure than the other rarely involved pathogenic 
sequelae. Such a conflict might involve a choice at the end of a meal between 
two types of desserts, one of which was much more highly cathected than 
the other. 

It is perhaps less obvious that a conflict between two high pressure forces 
evenly balanced represents the most pathological condition. It has been 
assumed by psychoanalysis that weakening the forces of repression will permit 
the neurotic to admit repressed wishes into consciousness, give cathartic expres- 



j6 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

sion to their pent-up energy, and eventually enable the individual to integrate 
these wishes with his adult personality and restore internal peace. But psycho- 
analytic practice employs such a procedure only in the case of the neuroses. 
It is contraindicated in prepsychotic conditions. In these conditions psycho- 
analysis admittedly may precipitate a psychotic state, and if this seems probable 
analysis is promptly terminated. We would explain this difference in the 
efficacy of psychoanalytic therapy in neurotic and prepsychotic conditions in 
the following way. In the case of the neuroses, the combined pressure of the 
opposed forces is less than the pressure available for the total personality, and 
the repressing force is relatively stronger than that which is repressed. In 
the prepsychotic condition the combined pressure of repressing and repressed 
force represents almost all of the pressure available to the individual, and the 
opposing forces maintain a more unstable equilibrium. The repressing force 
is only slightly stronger than the repressed force. If the repressed element is 
then admitted into consciousness by psychoanalytic intervention which weakens 
the force of repression, there is civil war within the personality. But in the 
neuroses there is a condition more akin to that of a revolutionary party which 
has been driven underground by a frightened democracy. It might be possible 
for the democratic society to tolerate and assimilate its own revolutionary 
minority, but not if the revolutionary element represented almost half the 
country. Originally repression of one of the forces drives it underground, 
but, as Freud showed us, an incessant guerrila warfare is maintained, and 
the individual must maintain constant vigilance lest the repressed element 
break forth again. But the severity of this guerrila warfare is clearly a function 
of the relative strength of the two forces. In the neuroses the repressed element 
is a minority party, and for this reason a relatively small army will be suf- 
ficient to contain this threat. But in the prepsychotic condition the personality 
is split in two, and the repressing force is not much more representative of the 
personality, or much stronger, than the repressed force. Consequently any 
alteration of the distribution of these forces may precipitate a psychotic episode. 
The psychoses are characteristically more labile in both inception and remission 
than the neuroses. 

Perhaps the least plausible of our derivations is that concerning the type 
of conflict in which the repressing force is very much greater than the repressed 
force. We have been reminded so often of the pathological sequelae of an 
overly strict super-ego that our hypothesis requires considerable clinical evi- 
dence if it is to be seriously entertained. In general we would maintain that 
an overly strict super-ego can result in symptom formation only if its pressure 
is matched by a repressed wish of almost equal pressure; that if the pressure 
of the repressed wish is much less than that of the repressing force, there will 
not be sufficient counterpressure to produce symptoms. Clinical evidence for 
this hypothesis will be presented in a later section. 



LEVEL ANALYSIS 77 

3. THE PROBLEM OF MEASUREMENT OF REPRESSION 

Having defined our units of analysis and proposed a set of hypotheses 
concerning the dynamics of repression, let us now consider the empirical 
problem of measurement of these forces. 

The measurement of the absolute and relative strength of two forces, one 
of which represses the other, is peculiarly difficult because the nature of the 
repressed force can be investigated only when the pressure of the repressing 
force has been reduced to zero. Let us assume that there are a number of 
individuals who are hostile to their fathers but who are overtly submissive 
because of fear of their fathers. Suppose then we were to experimentally 
weaken this fear by giving these individuals equivalent amounts of alcohol. 
We might find that one individual remained quite submissive in feeling 
and behavior toward his father, another felt hostile but did not express his 
hostility, another might express his hostility verbally to his father, and another 
might murder his father. There are two possible interpretations of this 
phenomenon: the first is that all of these individuals wished to murder their 
fathers but the pressure of their fear varied and alcohol, which was sufficient 
to release the inhibitions of one individual, was not enough to free the others. 
A second and equally plausible interpretation is that the pressure of their 
repressed hostility varied that the alcohol effectively removed the repressing 
fear, and the differences in degree of hostility expressed under alcohol faithfully 
represented the degree of hostility that had been repressed. To determine which 
of these alternatives was true would involve reducing the strength of the fear 
to a zero quantity, and then the unfettered behavior would indicate the pres- 
sure of the hostility. An estimate o the pressure of the fear could be achieved 
by measuring the force necessary to reduce the fear to a zero quantity. One 
of the principal obstacles to such measurement is our ignorance of techniques 
of reducing the strength of repressing forces to a zero quantity. In our 
present state of knowledge, quite apart from the problem of manipulating 
human subjects as scientific guinea pigs, we could never be sure that we had 
reduced this force to a zero quantity. Unless we were sure we had done this, 
we could never measure the pressure of the repressed wish; there would always 
be the possibility that if we weakened the super-ego a bit more something 
else might appear in overt behavior. Nor could we measure the strength 
of the super-ego unless we were certain when we had reduced it to a zero 
quantity, since it is defined by the pressure it exerts against repressed wishes. 

a) The Use of the TAT as an Indicator of Repression 

If these are some of the problems inherent in the experimental investiga- 
tion of repression, in what way can we utilize the TAT to estimate the pressure 
of repressing and repressed wishes? 



><8 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

It is our belief that the analysis of the individual's imaginative produc- 
tions does allow a more satisfactory measure of these forces than we can at 
present achieve through experimental investigation. We believe this is so 
because the strength of repressive factors is weakened by allowing the indi- 
vidual to achieve distance between himself and the characters of his stories* 
If we were to ask the individual to tell us what he would do if he were the 
person in the picture the repressive forces would be alerted and the privacy 
of inner thoughts guarded. The writer has investigated the effect of changing 
the instructions in this way and found that subjects either refuse to comply 
with these instructions or offer meager, impoverished stories or consciously 
attempt to confuse the examiner. 

In addition to the distance which the test itself places between the story- 
teller and his stories, the nature of the picture contributes distance. The second 
ten pictures, which are more bizarre and unrealistic, were chosen to increase 
this distance. Such pictures permit the individual license in telling stories 
unfettered by realistic considerations. In such a setting the subject can tell 
"crazy" stories with the feeling that they may be appropriate to the picture. 
Further, he is encouraged to do this by the change of instructions at the 
beginning of the second session that he may give freer rein to his imagina- 
tion. For these reasons the individual may attribute wishes to his heroes 
which no amount of alcohol or other experimental manipulation might induce 
him to express in overt behavior. 

But because the force of repression is only weakened and not reduced to 
a zero quantity, repressed wishes are usually not given completely free expres- 
sion even under these conditions. These wishes are given somewhat freer 
expression only in those stories in which the conditions are sufficiently distant 
from the everyday life of the individual to permit such license. We must 
therefore employ the technique of comparing the behavior of those heroes who 
are interacting "normally" with parents or others with those heroes who are 
represented in a setting psychologically "remote" from normal interaction. 

Let us consider some examples. If the TAT of an individual portrayed 
a son submissive to his father and to every other male figure in all his stories 
but one, and in this story a hero under the influence of alcohol, and in pre- 
historic times, destroyed some physical property in a blind rage, we would 
assume that the pressure of the repressing force was very great. We would 
assume this since it forbade the expression of any aggression except under 
the most unusual and remote conditions of this story and then only toward 
a nonliving object. If the same individual had also given us a story of a hero 
who attacked the prehistoric monster of card n, we would have said that 
the pressure of repression was somewhat less, since it permitted aggression 



LEVEL ANALYSIS 79 

toward living organisms.* If in addition the hero had rescued a princess from 
this monster, we would suppose that there was somewhat less repression, 
inasmuch as a human being had been introduced as the reason for this 
aggression. If the individual had portrayed all his heroes submissive in 
face-to-face relationships but in one story the hero led a revolt against the 
government, we would assume that the repressing force had been still weaker 
because it sanctioned aggression against human agencies. If in addition 
there had been stories of the hero's hatred of policemen, we would assume 
still less pressure of the repressing force because the impersonality of authority 
had been concretized in the person of the policeman. Less repression would 
have permitted some heroes to aggress upon other adult males with whom 
they had a more personal relationship. Finally, if the individual had also 
told stories in which the hero behaved submissively towards his father in 
the face-to-face relationship, but nursed private and unexpressed wishes for 
revenge, we would assume that the pressure of the repressing fear operated 
only to prevent the overt expression of the wish but did not prevent awareness 
of the wish. 

It is our assumption that the remoteness of conditions under which anti- 
social wishes may be expressed is a function of the relative pressure of repressing 
and repressed forces. As the repressing force increases in pressure relative 
to the pressure of the repressed force, the conditions under which the latter 
may be given expression in TAT stories become more and more "remote." 
As this ratio approaches equality the expression of the repressed force will 
appear under less and less remote conditions in the stories. 

Since we will employ "remoteness" as an index of repression, let us con- 
sider in more detail the varieties of remoteness. There is remoteness of object 
the wish may be directed toward a parent who is usually the original object 
of the repressed wish, or toward a parent surrogate, policemen, the law, gov- 
ernment, animals, or physical objects. This represents a typical series of 
increasingly remote objects for the displacement of repressed wishes. There 
is remoteness of time the present, the immediate past or future, the remote 
past or future. There is remoteness of setting the individual's customary 
habitat or geographically remote settings, ranging from other countries to 
other planets. There may be remoteness of level, ranging from behavior to 
wish, memories, daydreams, nigh td reams, and special states. Finally there 
may be remoteness of conditions ranging from the heroes' everyday conditions 

*Renaud (74) found that responses to card n by head-injury cases were distinguished 
by the life and death struggle between the hero and the monster, but that aggression 
expressed in the other stories was neither more nor less intense than in the control groups. 
This would be consistent with the theory that these individuals harbor intense hostility 
bound by repressive forces and that the pressure of this hostility is sufficient to call for 
intrapunitive injury because of guilt. 



80 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

to states o extreme fatigue, frustration, or anxiety, and so on. A story may 
be extremely remote in one respect but not in another. For example, normal 
individuals occasionally murder their parents in their TAT stories. This 
represents a minimum of remoteness o object but some other dimensions 
will usually be extremely remote the individual will depict either the parent 
or the hero as suffering some special condition. The parent may be repre- 
sented as particularly brutal because he is under the influence of alcohol, or 
the hero may be particularly frustrated or " queer." 

By means of these criteria we may estimate, crudely, differences in remote- 
ness of expression of repressed wishes between one protocol and another. If 
we were able to measure with any precision the degree of remoteness and 
the pressure of repressing force and repressed force as these appeared in TAT 
stories, we would be able to give an empirical value to the functional relation- 
ship which we have said obtains between these two forces and the degree of 
remoteness of expression. Since we lack a scale of measurement with a zero 
point and equal intervals we will have to await the further refinement of 
our scaling methods to determine more accurately the dynamics of repres- 
sion. But we can estimate somewhat crudely whether one force is greater 
than another and whether one expression of a repressed wish is more remote 
than another. We will now consider how this may be done, and how we 
may use such estimates in clinical diagnosis. 

It is necessary first to estimate the ratio of the combined press .i-e of 
repressing and repressed wish to the total pressure available to the personality. 
This may be done by totaling the number of stories which refer either to 
the repressing force or the repressed force and computing the percentage of 
such stories to the total number of stories.* In our previous example of an 
Oedipus conflict we might have found that every story involved either a son 
who was submissive to his father or a hero who expressed aggression under 
more remote conditions. If this were the case, we would estimate that the 
combined pressure of repressing and repressed forces was 100 per cent of the 
total pressure. But if only half of the stories were concerned with this con- 
flict, we would estimate a conflict of medium pressure, or 50 per cent. If only 
two stories referred to this conflict, we would assume a conflict of low pressure 
or 10 per cent. This is of course a very crude estimate of the total pressure 
of the conflict, but we have found it useful clinically in that the more seriously 
disturbed the individual the more frequently the protocol is weighted with 
such conflict throughout the stories. Less disturbed individuals have more 
stories free of this type of conflict. 

* This assumes that the set of 20 stories is a representative sample of the total available 
pressure an assumption demonstrably untrue in the case of some protocols. To what extent 
this assumption is generally true requires further empirical investigation. 



LEVEL ANALYSIS 8l 

We must next determine die ratio of repressing to repressed force. We 
have said before that experimental manipulation of the individual was limited 
by the fact that we could never be certain whether the force of repression had 
been reduced to a zero quantity hence we would be equally uncertain of the 
extent of the repressed wish. The TAT offers a unique opportunity for 
estimating the nature and pressure of a wish which has been repressed, if we 
examine the wish when allowed its freest expression in the TAT. This will 
generally be found under those conditions which are most remote. It may be 
argued that we cannot be certain that the forces of repression have been suf- 
ficiently reduced in the TAT to allow any more certainty than might be 
achieved through experimental de-inhibition. Such a criticism might be justi- 
fied, but there is reason to believe that such uncertainty has been much 
reduced, because normal individuals do give expression to extremely antisocial 
wishes in their TAT stories. The same individuals may exhibit none of these 
wishes either in their everyday behavior or when under the influence of 
alcohol. The same individuals may be completely unaware that such wishes 
attributed to their heroes are in any way related to their own personalities. 

Since we do not possess an equal interval scale for the measurement of 
psychological forces, the assignment of numbers to such variables as remoteness 
and the pressure of repressed and repressing wish cannot be more than a 
metaphorical venture. Assignment of numbers under such conditions might 
indicate that one quantity was greater than another but would not indicate 
precisely how much greater. For this reason these quantities could not be 
manipulated mathematically. We must therefore await the development of 
more precise scales of measurement before we can undertake a precise quanti- 
tative formulation of the dynamics of repression. If we possessed such meas- 
uring techniques the diagnosis of the degree of repression would be a rela- 
tively simple matter. 

If we cannot measure these variables accurately we may yet attempt 
crude estimates of their strength. For our purposes four measures are needed: 
the degree of remoteness of expression, the pressure of repressing wish, the 
pressure of repressed wish, and the ratio of the combined pressure of repressing 
wish and repressed wish to total pressure. The latter ratio may be- estimated 
by the number of stories mentioning either the repressing or repressed wish. 
This number can then be expressed as a percentage of the total number of 
stories. 

The degree of remoteness may be estimated roughly as very high, high, 
medium, low, and very low according to criteria previously discussed. 

The pressure of the repressed wish may also be estimated on the same 
scale: very high, high, medium, low, and very low. Since this pressure is the 
product of the intensity and extensity of the repressed wish, these must first be 



82 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

estimated before pressure can be determined. We must rate the intensity 
and extensity of the repressed wish as -it appears in each story estimate the 
product of both components and then add these separate estimates to achieve 
a total estimate of the pressure of the repressed wish. Let us suppose that 
an individual told only one story in which there was expression of an anti- 
social wish of high intensity and high extensity and that another individual 
told the same kind of story, but also told 10 other stories in which the same 
wish was expressed although of lower intensity and lower extensity. The 
estimate of pressure of the repressed wish in the first case would be low and 
in the second case high. Although the first individual told a story giving 
expression to a wish of high intensity and high extensity, the fact that this 
was the only story of this kind would indicate that its total pressure was lower 
than the pressure of the wish of the second individual who told an additional 
10 stories expressing the same wish even though the wish was at lower 
intensity and extensity in each of the additional stories. 

In order to estimate the pressure of the repressing force, we would employ 
our hypothesis that remoteness is a function of the ratio of repressing pressure 
to repressed pressure. Having determined the pressure of the repressed wish 
and the degree of remoteness, the estimated product of these two estimates 
provides an estimate of the pressure of the repressing force. Thus, if remoteness 
were high and pressure of the repressed wish were high, the estimated pressure 
of the repressing force would be very high. If remoteness were very high and 
pressure of the repressed wish medium, the estimated pressure of the repressing 
wish would be high. We have not -attempted quantification of these relation- 
ships because measurement is only crudely approximated. But although these 
suggested operations of estimation leave much to be desired, they may nonethe- 
less yield operationally testable hypotheses, as we shall see in the application 
of these techniques to case material. 

Before turning to the case material, let us consider briefly the problem 
of identifying the repressed and repressing forces. In general almost any wish 
may be repressed by any other wish. Usually these wishes are "antisocial," 
but the definition of what is antisocial varies from culture to culture and from 
individual to individual within a culture as heterogeneous as ours. The 
interpreter must, in his assessment of repressed wishes, know the larger cul- 
tural pressures and the more particular pressures to which the individual has 
been subjected. The TAT protocol itself will provide some of the clues in such 
an assessment: wishes which occasion the hero anxiety, shame, embarrassment, 
remorse, or guilt or which the hero tries to conceal from others or which lead 
to his punishment these are wishes which may in that individual suffer 
repression. 



LEVEL ANALYSIS 83 

4. THE CASES OF X, Y, AND Z 

We shall now present in some detail 3 cases X, Y, and Z to illustrate 
our hypotheses concerning the nature of repression. Each of the TAT's 
revealed that aggression against a parent had been repressed by these indi- 
viduals. In X's case this wish and its control expended much of the available 
energy, representing a high pressure, medium pressure conflict with low 
remoteness of expression of aggression. In Y's case the combined pressure 
of repressing force and aggression was high. The repressing force was of high 
pressure, but the aggression was of very low pressure, having high intensity 
but very low extensity. Remoteness of expression of aggression was high. 
In Z this conflict consumed less energy than in X or Y. It represented a high 
pressure, low pressure conflict. Remoteness of expression of aggression repre- 
sented a mid-point between the other cases, being of medium degree. We 
shall also examine the extent to which each of these individuals was disturbed. 
X was most seriously disturbed, Y least disturbed, and Z represented a mid- 
point between the two. Let us consider first the case of X. 

a) The Case of X 

X is a young women who describes her father as a quite refined man 
with many gifts and a fine personality. She is admittedly much fonder of her 
father than her mother, but feels sorry for her mother because her life has been 
a hard one and X feels her mother deserved a better fate. It is evident from 
her stories that her hostility toward her mother is marked. In the face-to-face 
situation her heroine feels this hostility, but suppresses it. 

This woman is trying to talk some sense into this girl. She is reading a 
preceptual passage to her but the girl's mind is far away. She does not 
want to listen to this woman (her mother) because she associates most of her 
previous humiliating experiences with her. She wants to get away, to be free. 
She has mixed feelings of self pity, aggression, and a desire to start out on 
her own. However she makes an effort and suppresses this mood and listens 
to what her mother is reading. 

On the basis of this story we would expect to find further evidence of 
aggression directed toward other females and would predict that remote 
displacement would not be required, since the existence of hostility is freely 
admitted into the consciousness of the heroine in the face-to-face situation. 

In the following story we see the transference of this affect to other 
women. 

This woman is jealous of the other woman because she has a sweeter dis- 
position, etc., etc., and men seem to prefer the company of woman two 
because she is so gay and charming, even though not one fifth as intelligent 



84 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

as woman one. "I don't care" says woman one and returns to her book. 
Little by little the woman becomes absorbed in her book and the petty 
jealousies, etc., seem to fall off. In fact when she puts the book aside she 
feels very friendly toward the whole world and even goes out of her way 
to be good to woman two. 

There is a continuation of hostility and jealousy toward other females, 
and the reasons are similar she feels that she is inferior to her mother and 
other women in competition for the attention of men. We are told in addi- 
tion that she has learned to control this aggression, which she could do no 
more than suppress in childhood, by turning to books, which enables the petty 
jealousies to "fall off." In the following story there is the same attempt 
to manage the aggression born of frustration and feelings of inferiority by 
immersing herself in something which removes her from her own bitterness. 

That hideous woman at the back is this woman's evil nature which she has 
long suppressed. This woman wasn't really evil when a child but things 
went against her. She felt frustrated on every hand. Now however she 
wants to restore her own mental health by attaching herself to some great 
and selfless cause. The more she strives in this direction the fainter and 
fainter grows the woman at the back. Eventually the finer qualities in her 
are completely reinstated. 

These are the conditions of her mental health, identification with and 
dedication to something which will make the aggression grow "fainter and 
fainter." This mechanism must be distinguished from reaction formation. 
She does not "pretend" to be more friendly than she really is. In the previous 
story she "went out of her way" to be friendly towards the woman she hated 
because she had been able to overcome her aggression through absorption in 
something which interested her, and this diminished the feelings of inferiority 
which aroused her aggression. In this story her attachment to a noble cause 
is the instrument of dissipating the aggression and inferiority at the same time. 
As a result the aggression fades. It can be reduced in intensity and extensity 
because it is not an end per se but the consequence of her intolerable feeling 
of humiliation and inferiority. For this reason reading, in one case, and 
dedication to a selfless cause, in the other, can at once dissipate both the feeling 
of inferiority and the aggression which results from it. 

We are also told, indirectly, how her mental health might be irreparably 
shattered. If her feelings of inferiority were for any reason to increase beyond 
a critical point, with no possibility of overcoming diem, we might predict 
that she would be overwhelmed by the aggression which would result from 
her humiliation. This happens to one of her heroines and the aggression is 
directed against the original object, the mother. 



LEVEL ANALYSIS 85 

The woman who is strangling the other woman is crazy. She used to be 
a pretty girl once but she developed a physical deformity her hand became 
swollen and ugly. She was so distressed by this fact that her whole per. 
sonality changed. She felt nobody cared for her any more. She became 
morose and suspicious of others even when they were being genuinely nice 
to her. Little by little this neurotic trend grew till she became positively 
dangerous. Her mother loved her and wanted to protect her so she would 
not hear of the girl being sent to the asylum. However in one of her fits 
the maniac caught her mother by the throat as she was coming down the 
stairs, and strangled her. The rest of the family rushed in but it was too 
late. They did not punish her of course but they put her in a mental home. 

The presence of an inferiority which cannot be overcome results in a 
feeling of humiliation and suspicion of the attitude of others toward her, 
which leads ultimately to an overwhelming "fit" of aggression against her 
mother. 

That the joint problems of inferiority and resultant aggression are the 
principal concern of this young woman is further indicated by the following 
three stories. 

This woman is pleading with this man to leave his work for a while and 
relax. She offers herself to him. The man here seems to be hesitating 
between his duty and his love, but actually he knows which really matters 
to him. Besides he will soon grow tired of the woman and the temporary 
pleasure is nothing compared with the rewards of his work. So he fools 
around with her for a while and then throws her off without a remorse 
because after all she got what she wanted, and he gave as much as was his 
to give. The woman takes it badly at first but she recovers and throws 
herself into her own work with greater determination. She is thrilled to 
find how well she makes out on her job. 

This woman is blissfully happy because she has found someone who returns 
her affection with equal intensity. The man is here a little amused at her 
childish clinging because he had thought her so mature and self-contained. 
She on her part is happy because she can cling to him as much as she likes 
without fear of his ceasing to respect her on that score; she wants to feel 
thus protected and cherished always so that she might in turn be a source 
of strength and faith. A year ago she despaired of ever meeting such a 
person. Now she laughs because she had doubted her destiny. The two 
work together and make great contributions to the welfare of humanity. 
They are a sort of combination of Einstein and Madame Curie. 

This woman is afraid that her husband is not fond of her any more. She 
feels that she has failed to come up to his expectations even though he has 
not said anything to that effect; he is at present sitting in an armchair, reading 
a newspaper. She steps into the room and feels frustrated by his absorption 
in the paper. She tries to break in through his reading with some totally 
unintelligent remark. As he looks up she feels he despises her because she 



86 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

is so obviously asking for attention. She feels that by her foolish behavior on 
this and other occasions she has lost his esteem and is mad both at herself 
and at him. I don't know how such a story would end. She would have 
to take herself in hand I guess. 

In these stories the conditions necessary for her happiness in love and 
work are presented. She can be happy if she can be dependent without 
fear of losing self respect or the respect of her lover. Through the secure 
gratification of this dependency she is enabled to offer something in return, 
and the couple working together make great contributions to the welfare of 
humanity. But if the superior male rejects her and makes her feel inferior 
she will turn to work to escape her misery. She is "thrilled to find how well 
she makes out on her job" but she does not make "great contributions to the 
welfare of humanity." Nor is she a "Madame Curie." 

This young women suffered acute feelings of inferiority and rage and 
much of her energy was expended in coping with these feelings, controlling 
the aggression she felt, repressing its more intense and primitive components. 
Evidence from the TAT concerning the combined pressure of repressing and 
repressed wishes is congruent with this fact. Approximately 75 per cent of 
her stories are concerned either with the repressing force (the wish to be 
respected and loved) and the repressed wish (the wish to aggress upon those 
who humiliated her). Approximately 40 per cent of all the stories express 
the latter wish with varying degrees of intensity, consequently we estimated 
the pressure of aggression as medium:. We have rated the remoteness of 
expression as low since she is aware of these feelings in the face-to-face situa- 
tion with her mother, although she suppresses the overt expression of these 
feelings. She continues to be aware -of these feelings throughout the stories, 
although the most intense aggression is expressed under more remote condi- 
tions by a "maniac" in a "fit." We have rated this remoteness as low (but 
not very low) because of the continuity of the expression of aggression from 
face-to-face normal conditions and remote conditions. The repressing force 
was estimated as a force of high (but not very high) pressure the product 
of low remoteness and medium pressure of the repressed wish (the product 
of any multiplication is necessarily higher than either multiplier, unless one 
multiplier has a value of one or less). 

According to our previous hypotheses, such a constellation of forces is 
approximately midway between a neurotic and prepsychotic state, since much 
but not all of the individual's energies are consumed in conflict (75 per cent). 
If less energy had been consumed it would have been nearer the neurotic 
type of conflict. If more energy were expended it would have been nearer 
the prepsychotic state. Moreover the ratio of repressed to repressing forces, 
although not equal (high pressure against medium pressure) is sufficiently near 



LEVEL ANALYSIS 87 

equality to provide an unstable equilibrium of forces. In such a case the 
individual's mental health is capable of being shattered through the intensifica- 
tion of the strength of the repressed force. As in the story told by X, she 
might lose her sanity if she suffered sufficient increase in her feelings of 
inferiority to intensify the aggression which results from intolerable humilia- 
tion. She might, however, become a "Madame Curie" if she found a man 
who made her feel both loved and respected. Under these conditions she 
would be capable not only of trust and dependence, but she could in addition 
offer nurturance to such a 'man, and together they would make great contribu- 
tions to the welfare of mankind. This case illustrates very clearly one of the 
neglected aspects of neurosis the unstable equilibrium characteristic of certain 
conflicts. Although her suppressed aggressive wish presents a real problem 
and might conceivably produce a psychotic state, it is not in and of itself the 
primary problem. It is a resultant force the resultant of feeling inferior 
and unloved by the father surrogate. For this reason the vicissitudes of her love 
and work can, as they vary from day to day, intensify or completely do away 
with her feelings of aggression, so that the relationship which we have assumed 
to exist between the repressing force and the repressed wish is true only 
under certain specific conditions of frustration. When these are increased or 
decreased the relationship between repressed aggression and repressing force 
may change radically. A necessary condition of an unstable equilibrium 
between two psychological forces is a delicate balance of their respective pres- 
sures. If this is the case, an important corollary may be drawn: that such 
individuals would be most sensitive to therapy which was directive or which 
attempted manipulation of the patient's environment. Placing such individuals 
in an environment optimal for their specific needs could reduce conflicts which 
might otherwise be recalcitrant to therapy and which might under adverse 
environmental conditions result in serious impairment of their mental health. 

b) The Case of Y 

Y is a young woman, 20 years of age, whose attitudes as they were 
expressed in interviews were not dissimilar to those of X. Y adores her 
father, feels much less tenderness toward her mother, although she tries to be 
fair, "I realize that she's that way and apparently cannot overcome it so I think 
nothing of it." There are, however, important differences: Y's father recipro- 
cates her feelings, "He is especially friendly towards me and is happy if I am 
happy." "The respect which my father has for me is very great and to me 
this is very important because unless I am worthy of his respect we could not 
be close friends as we are." This latter is reminiscent of X's need to be 
respected by the idealized father who elicits her respect. 

Despite the great similarity of the basic personalities of X and Y, the 



88 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

differences are profound. Y has achieved the relationship which might save 
X from being overwhelmed by humiliation and rage. Y's worship of her 
father has not interfered with her plans for marriage. She has transferred 
to her future husband the adoration which she has felt towards her father 
and seems destined to achieve a happy marriage. 

The TAT stories told by her are what we might have expected. In the 
following story she tells us again of her adoration of her father. 

Frank's father was a well known violinist and Frank worshiped him. His 
main ambition in life was to be a great musician, as proficient as his father, 
but he doubted if anyone else could be so talented. He has had only a very 
few lessons and sits, pondering over the violin. Frank cannot visualize just 
how such beautiful music can come from such an instrument and wonders 
whether he should give the whole idea up or if, after many years, he could 
attain the goal he wants to set for himself. He concluded that even though 
he's young he is somewhat like his father and he too can be a musician. 

In her second story her somewhat ambivalent but not overly intense feelings 
toward her mother are delineated. 

Ethel had always gone to a country school and was interested in getting an 
education. Ethel's mother had very little education but was happy with what 
she had been accustomed to and could not understand why Ethel must have 
more than a high school education. Ethel does not like to go against her 
mother's wishes but tries to explain that many changes are taking place in 
the world today and that she cannot be satisfied with the insignificant life she 
had led thus far and she wants to go to school, get an education and see 
what is really going on in the world. 

Finally, the transference of her love from her father to her future husband, 
as well as her fulfillment in maternity, is expressed in a wish fulfillment 
phantasy. 

Mary and John had been married just a year ago, before he joined the 
Navy. He had been home on leave two months ago and was now at sea. 
Mary loves him deeply and naturally misses him but tries to keep her 
mind occupied and not to worry about him. On this particular night, how- 
ever, she's restless and cannot sleep. She tries reading but can think of 
nothing but Johnny. She walks out in the hall and stands by the window, 
dreaming of Johnny and the day he'll be back. Mary doesn't feel well the 
next morning and goes to the Doctor, to learn that she is going to have a 
baby. After this she is very busy, making preparations for the baby and the 
time passes very quickly. Johnny gets another leave and is with Mary when 
the baby is born. 

These three stories are typical of the entire protocol and indicate that 
there is no discrepancy between her overt behavior, publicly expressed attitudes 
and her private world. She is not an individual divided against herself, or 



LEVEL ANALYSIS 89 

one whose behavior exemplifies any pathogenic mechanism which we could 
detect. She is entirely free of neurotic symptom formation and anxiety. 
Her stones were all like this, with the one exception which follows. 

As much as Richard disliked it, he was becoming quite accustomed to having 
his father come home in a drunken condition but 'this evening when, in 
addition to being highly inebriated, he was angry and started beating his 
wife, it was too much for Richard, who was very fond of his mother. Richard 
tried to control his temper but with no avail. He was certain that his father 
did no good for his family or anyone and before he realized just what he 
was doing, he reached for the revolver and shot him. Richard has now 
decided that even though he must give himself up, it is much better this way 
than having his mother tortured the way she has been for the past two years. 

The murder of parents in TAT stories is an extremely rare phenomenon 
in "normals." Had we found any evidence of such a wish we would have 
expected it to be directed against the mother, although there were no indica- 
tions of hostility of such proportions. That this story could be told by an 
individual who loves both her father and future husband so deeply, and is 
apparently so free of ambivalence is very puzzling when one is accustomed 
to expect all asocial wishes which suffer repression to press toward expression 
and to produce symptoms if they are not expressed. To the best of our 
knowledge this story is an isolated fragment in the total picture of an otherwise 
well adjusted individual. What it represents one can only guess. It is con- 
ceivably a residue of accidentally witnessing the primal scene, seeing the father 
who was distinguished for his kindness and even temper excited and passionate 
and apparently hurting the mother. But whatever its meaning it is clear that 
the father is seen to be different from his normal self. He is pictured under 
the influence of alcohol, and the aggression which he displays is given a finite 
course two years. Presumably this represented a change of character in the 
father. Consonant with our hypothesis that this fragment produces no 
pressure symptoms is the hero's lack of remorse for this murder. 

We have said that Y's conflict was between a force of high pressure 
and very low pressure and that the remoteness of expression of aggression was 
high. The combined pressure of the repressing force adoration of the father 
and the wish to be like him and be loved by him and the repressed wish to 
kill him, is equivalent to that in the case of X approximately 75 per cent 
of the stories are devoted to one or the other of these two wishes. But in this 
case we have rated the pressure of repressed aggression as very low because 
this aggression appears in only one story. Although its intensity is very high 
in this story the total estimate of pressure (based on its intensity and extensity 
throughout the protocol) would be very low, since there is no other evidence 
of the wish. We would rate the remoteness of expression as high since there 



go THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

is no continuity between the face-to-face normal relationship with the father 
and the murder of the father under remote conditions. We would have rated 
it as very high if the hero had instead murdered an animal or an adult other 
than the father. The pressure of the repressing force we would rate as high 
the product of high remoteness and very low pressure (as if we had multiplied 
a high degree of remoteness by one). 

We have said before that a conflict between a high and very low pressure 
wish should not permit a return of the repressed wish or sequelae which are 
pathogenic. This hypothesis would appear to be supported by the evidence 
from this case. The positive love of the father and future husband exerts 
sufficient pressure to contain this repressed fragment of hostility and at the 
same time permits the individual no awareness of its presence. It is as remote 
from the personality as it is isolated within the microcosm of the TAT. 

c) The Case of Z 

Z is a young man of 19 who was studied intensively over a ten month 
period. He was given four successive administrations of the TAT at three 
month intervals. He was also presented with other pictures daily. He told 
over 400 stories during this period. 

Not unlike X and Y, Z was also much possessed by the family romance. 
He had, in many respects, a classical Oedipus conflict. He loved his mother 
dearly, respected his father and was deferent to him. The TAT revealed 
aggression toward his father less repressed than in the case of Y, but more 
repressed than in the case of X. 

Let us examine first those stories in which we see the hero in a face-to-face 
relationship with his father. 

This fellow has had amnesia and they are now taking his measurements. By 
these measurements they will discover that he is the son of a wealthy man. 
He goes to live with his father who is a sadist and because of the treatment 
he runs away, getting another attack. 

The son is submissive to the sadistic father and runs away, to suffer 
another attack of amnesia. In the next story we are told of the enduring 
consequence of his father's severity. 

These children have been told by their father definitely not to leave the 
house. Their friends are at the window coaxing them to come out and play. 
They shake their heads but after a long while sneak out. They are 
caught and severely punished and this is the way their childhood is spent. 
When they grow up they become strict disciplinarians, except the boy who 
will be very gentle, almost effeminate. 

The consequence of this severe paternal discipline is that the boy "will 
be very gentle, almost effeminate." This again is the relationship in the face- 



LEVEL ANALYSIS 9! 

toface situation. But as the pictures and stories lead into general social rela- 
tionships and away from the family this picture changes. 

This picture is supposed to represent a person hypnotizing another. This 
person is an older fellow sitting there. He is an insane person. He has 
great illusions about himself. He thinks he can cause the will of another 
to snap into his own will and make him do whatever he wants. This person 
has gone to sleep and pays no attention. This upsets him and he goes back 
to the insane asylum. 

The father surrogate is still portrayed as an omnipotent figure but he is 
insanely so, and the younger man refuses to comply with his wishes. There 
is here no overt aggression or even overt rebelliousness but a passive resistance 
he has "gone to sleep and pays no attention." 

As the stories increase in remoteness there is a gradual change in the 
hero's reactions. 

This old guy. I hate him. He is the most disgusting individual. He is a 
bourgeois capitalist and he spies on his friends, sees things they don't want 
him to see. His life is not complete unless he observes his friends unaware. 
Such a low individual has been completely summed up. Somebody will 
catch him spying and shoot him through the heart. 

In this story the older male adult is a more remote object of displaced 
aggression, inasmuch as he is seen as a representative of a class of men who 
are rejected. The hero's reactions have as a function of this increased distance 
changed from passive resistance to a feeling of hatred, but there is still no 
overt aggression. The father surrogate is still portrayed as someone who 
exercises too much dominance over the lives of others and who "spies on 
his friends." Although the hero does not express his hatred, the story ends 
with the possibility that "somebody will catch him spying and shoot him 
through the heart." This is the first act of overt aggression against a father 
surrogate, although it is not the hero's doing. 

With more remoteness, there is a change to overt aggression. 

This man has been blinded by tear gas, and is now being led by a friend 
out of a group that is being dispersed by the police. His activities in this 
group were innocent; he now becomes a cop-hater. This may sometime lead 
him to get in an argument with a policeman and strike him. For this there 
would be a jail sentence and further rooting of his dislike for the police. 

This story represents one further step away from the father in that the 
policeman is not the representative of a class, but the representative of the law 
and society at large, and he envisions a possible overt expression of aggression 
towards policemen. In addition, the punishment for this act of aggressiqn 
will intensify his hatred. 



92 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

In this story he was "innocent" at the beginning. In the next story he is 
less innocent. 

The man in the picture is a laborer. He has just been to a labor agitation 
meeting. Now he is being forceably exited. The result of this is that he 
will become more antisocial than ever. He will become more stubborn, 
perhaps run up against the law and go to jail. Anyway you look at it this 
fellow's life will become less and less satisfactory to society. 

Here he becomes "more antisocial than ever" and "more stubborn." The 
implication is clear that he went to the labor agitation meeting in the first 
place with antisocial intent. The hero is still portrayed as the victim of 
persecution. The "law" and "society" are no less sadistic than the father, 
but we see him turning more and more openly against these paternal surro- 
gates. His role is becoming less and less passive. 

The following story represents the underlying wish displaced to the object 
at greatest remove from the father. 

The man is an instigator of a revolt. Having laid his plans he now has gone 
home and standing at the window with die room darkened he watches the 
explosion in a gov't building which is a signal for th- revolt to begin. 
Mingled emotions are experienced by him at this instant, fear for an instant 
then excitement and joy trust of his companions. 

This is the clearest expression of the underlying wish to get rid of the 
omnipotent and ubiquitous father. The object is remotely displaced. It is 
the impersonal force of authority represented by the established government. 
It is of further interest that this revolt is not punished. It would appear that 
the cooperation of trusted allies had made a successful holy war of this revolt. 
But although revolt may call for the cooperation of the oppressed, and 
although it may be successful against the "government" it is not so successful 
when the parent surrogate is a less remote and more concrete tyrant. 

An eclipse colored slaves are unloading a boat The wife of the foreman is 
on the bridge looking for the law for the men are bootlegging. The Negroes 
will take the eclipse as a sign and revolt and kill their master. The wife 
will run away, the slaves will be caught and sold again all because of the 
eclipse. 

Here the condition of revolt is the pact of slaves against the master. This 
is the most open aggression expressed towards the father figure, albeit at 
some distance, but although there is group responsibility for the act, the whole 
group is caught and sold again. This aggression is less remotely displaced 
insofar as the object of aggression is a "master" rather than the more imper- 
sonal government. 

If these feelings of aggression are as intense as they seem to be we would 



LEVEL ANALYSIS 93 

expect that the sadism embodied in the father's behavior toward the son might 
well engender sadistic impulses of revenge in the son. We have as yet seen 
no evidence of a purely sadistic enjoyment on the part of the hero. He has 
expressed hatred, he has struck a policeman, overthrown the government, and 
with others killed his master. We would assume that the conditions necessary 
for the full expression of this sadistic wish would be an extreme degree of 
remoteness of the displaced object of his aggression. Such is the case in the 
only story of its kind told to over 400 pictures. 

This man has just set fire to a stable full of horses, but he couldn't resist 
the temptation to stay around and watch the agony of the animals. While 
he was doing this a watchman catches him and he is taken to prison. 

This is at once the most remotely displaced object of his aggression and 
the most open expression of the depth of his feeling. The conditions of this 
story are remote in two senses. First, aggression is expressed towards animals 
rather than human beings, and second, the objects of his aggression are helpless 
victims who are incapable of counter aggression. Under these joint condi- 
tions of remoteness he can aggress, torture, and enjoy the agony of his victims. 

We should expect that any harm which befell the father would in no 
way be the responsibility of the son. The following story is an example of 
such a story. 

Bringing home the groceries a father slipped and sprained his ankle. It hurt 
and so he got into the bath tub and turned on the hot water. This caused 
increased swelling and the man lost two weeks from work besides much sleep. 

The father suffers an "accident," but the son is in no way responsible. 
Another story of the same variety follows: 

The man's son is leaving on the train after spending a week's furlough with 
him. This is the last time that the man will see his son. Within a week 
the man is dead. 

The father dies after seeing the son; but the son is by that time far away 
and is clearly not responsible for his father's death. The death of the father, 
for no apparent reason, is testament to the remoteness of the death wish from 
the individual's consciousness. 

In order to evaluate the pressure of this repressed wish we will have to 
consider at some length the other wishes expressed in his protocol. We are 
fortunate in finding in this individual another important wish which is also 
repressed. The comparison of two repressed wishes within the same indi- 
vidual will allow us a crucial test of our hypothesis concerning the dynamics 
of repression. 

There is much evidence for a repressed wish for the exclusive and com- 



94 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

plete possession of his mother. The fusion of the wish for love and sex from 
his mother has created serious problems. It has been further complicated 
by a fusion of sex and aggression. In the face-to-face relationship with the 
father we saw no evidence of any trace of the repressed wish of aggression. 
In the face-to-face relationship with the mother, there is evidence of his love 
for his mother in the following rescue thema. 

This woman heard someone in the living room and thinking it was her son 
and friends she has opened the door to say goodnight before she retires. 
Instead it turns out to be robbers who turn a gun on her and force her to tell 
where the valuables are. As they are leaving her son comes in and being an 
impetuous person attacks them. They shoot him and escape. She immediately 
goes to aid him. Does he die? No, it's only a severe flesh wound. 

This classical thema is apparently close enough to consciousness and of 
sufficient pressure to be projected into the mother-son relationship rather than 
toward some more displaced object. Individuals in whom this wish is more 
repressed express it under more remote conditions, such as a response to 
card ii about a prince who rescues the fairy princess from the dragon. 

More striking than this manifestation of his wish is the following story 
in which a fusion of sex and aggression is attributed to the mother: 

The person on the left is the boy's mother. While fondling the boy she all 
at once bit off part of the boy's ear. After this she became a perfect wreck 
and had to be separated from the boy, who became very afraid of her. 
Later she died in an insane asylum. 

We are told in effect that "fondling" between mother and son may lead 
to loss of control, in a fusion of sex and aggression, and this may lead to suicide. 

This story, with modifications, is repeated with the brother as the hero, 
rather than the mother. 

There has just been a murder committed. The man has killed his sister and 
dazed and stunned stumbles out and stubs his toe. Gangrene sets in and his 
toe is cut off. Then he is hung. 

This story was told to card 13, the picture of a naked woman in bed 
and a man standing near by. It is important in the interpretation of this 
story to know that this subject has no sister, but that he considers his mother 
more of a sister than a mother to him. From material which will be presented 
later, it is clear that this picture of a naked woman incited the same fusion 
of sex and aggression which seized the mother above, when she was fondling 
her son. It is no less dangerous an act for the son than the mother. The 
consequences are peculiarly severe he "stubs his toe. Gangrene sets in and 
his toe is cut off. Then he is hung." It is interesting to compare the punish- 
ment suffered by heroes who aggressed upon paternal surrogates and this 



LEVEL ANALYSIS 95 

punishment, which presumably stems from the same source. Aggression 
against father figures received relatively light punishment the slaves were 
caught and sold again for killing their master, the hero was jailed for his 
antisocial behavior toward the policemen and toward the horses, and when 
he revolted against the government there was no mention of punishment of 
any kind. But for the murder of his "sister," no single punishment is severe 
enough. And the consequence of similar behavior on the part of the mother 
is insanity. 

In another response to the same card in another administration of the 
test, he told the following story. 

The man is very drunk. In this coftdition he has gone to see the woman 
shown here. She has gotten undressed and into bed and is now pretending 
to pay no attention to him and to be disinterested. He is getting undressed 
in order to get into bed with her. In the moment shown, he has gained some 
of his faculties and is at this moment thinking how wrong it is to do the 
thing he has contemplated. Immediately, however, liquor will cloud his mind 
and his body will take control. Afterwards, he will probably be very upset 
about it. 

In this response, where the remoteness of the object and condition is 
increased he is "very drunk" he is able to consumate the sexual act. We 
see a third type of remoteness in this act in that he disclaims responsibility 
for it "immediately however liquor will cloud his mind and his body will 
take control." Concomitant with this increased remoteness, there is a decrease 
in the severity of punishment. He "will probably be very upset about it," but 
suffers nothing more severe. This is a page from the subject's past history. 
Previous to the period of testing he was involved in one such episode, very 
much under the influence of alcohol, and suffered remorse afterwards. 

His heroes are generally tortured by the problem of the control of their 
sexual impulses and the serious, consequences of the loss of such control. 

A typical example of this conflict appears in the following story. 

The scene is an English churchyard at night about a century ago The man is 
a clergyman and he is worshipping at the grave of a woman whom he loved 
deeply but never revealed his emotion. The struggle between celibacy and 
his natural desire has ruined his health. Now he is thanking God that the 
struggle is over and he can sublimate his passion by loving her memory. 

It is clear from this story that Z suffers no split in his libido. His sexual 
and love wishes are directed toward the same object. 

In the following story the typical sequel to actual loss of control is given. 

The man while suddenly kissing the girl on the cheek bit her cheek. He 
apologized profusely. He could [slip?! te H Wn 7 he had done it. No avail 
their acquaintanceship was broken off. 



g6 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

This story was written and the words "could tell why" probably represent 
a slip of the pencil, again indicating the pressure of the repre^ed wish and 
the relative weakness of the repressing force. We see again the fusion of 
oral aggression with sexuality that was first noted in the story of the mother 
who bit off part of her son's ear. The consequence of this loss of control 
is less serious than the loss of control on the part of the mother. The relation- 
ship is broken, but there is no insanity following loss of control. 

In the following story the same thema is repeated. 

This couple are dancing together. They have been old friends for a long 
time. Now the man tells the woman that he loves her. He kisses her 
lightly on the lips. Then she'll tell him that she doesn't love him and not to 
put her in a position where she'll have to forfeit his friendship by not 
allowing him to see her. 

It is of some interest that where the hero suffers either the disruption of 
the relationship, or external punishment, there is no guilt or remorse, and 
conversely, where the hero suffers guilt there is no punishment. But some 
form of punishment, either exogenous or endogenous, is present in all his love 
stories. 

In the following story there is an interesting denial of sexual wishes. 

The woman has just gone through a harrowing experience. She has come to 
tell her friend about it but at the memory of it she faints as is shown. That 
he is not making undesired advances to her is shown by the way he holds 
her. Of interest is the semicircle near his forehead, which may either be a 
lock of hair or part of the door, and also the picture which doesn't seem 
centered or significant in such a large frame. 

Notice that the statement "That he is not making undesired advances 
to her is shown by the way he holds her" creates sufficient anxiety to disrupt 
his story and lead him to describe small details of the picture. This was a 
rather typical response to anxiety which was incited by his own stories. We 
see again that the repressed sexual wish is sufficiently powerful to generate 
anxiety at the thought of it breaking through. Stories of his repressed aggres- 
sive need did not result in such small detail response to anxiety. 

Arising from the fact that his love object and his sex object are one and 
the same person and that his love and sex need have a passionate oral aggres- 
sive component, there is such a generalization of these repressed wishes to more 
remote love and sex objects that he cannot, in his stories, allow any con- 
summation of marriage. 

The following story is typical of this inhibition. 

The woman has heard some very sad news perhaps a person close to her 
has died. She turns to the man, a close friend, perhaps a lover and seeks 
consolation. ... In another way, depending on how the girl's eye interprets 



LEVEL ANALYSIS 97 

emotion to the observer, the girl might be dancing with the boy. She acts 
very sophisticated and he seems amused, perhaps at something she'd said. 
They will be good (not close) friends but won't ever marry. 

"Good" friends are typically not "close" friends. In the following story 
there is further evidence of the repressing force: 

Both these people are married [crossed out] working and they have made it a 
point to get time off together and go for a bicycle trip during the summer. 
This has now become almost a tradition and will go on for years. They are 
friends and never show any warm feelings for each other. 

He began the story about a married couple and then penciled over it to 
make them friends who "never show any warm feelings for each other." 
Again, good friends are not close friends. 

In the following story the same logic determines the nature of the 
relationship. 

These two people had been in love in their youth but had separated. The 
woman is a widow and now in their old age they enjoy each other's company 
calmly and dispassionately. 

Where there is mutual passion, the couple is separated, and allowed to 
reunite only after they are capable of a more calm relationship. 
Another example of the same thema is the following. 

These two children play together often. The little girl learns that her family 
is to move away. She tells the little boy nothing about this but kisses him 
good-bye. She joins a nunnery when she grows up and doesn't see the boy 
again until she is very old when she is visiting a dying man in the poor 
district. He smiles and she follows an impulse to kiss him although she 
does not recognize him. He begins to get better but then dies suddenly. 

Although there is the reunion here of two lovers who still are capable of 
passion, the man after a brief improvement "dies suddenly." 

Or, the couple may be separated by the premature death of one of the 
lovers. 

When a young boy, Roger Rollins brought his girl, Mary Caudry, to this tree 
and carved their initials on it. Later in her teens, Mary died and Roger 
every year makes a pilgrimage to this tree. 

The following two stories are typical of a series of similar stories, showing 
the destiny of those who are brave enough to marry. 

This couple has been traveling on their honeymoon and now they are tired 
of hotels and are talking about the new house they are going to move into 
next week. On the morrow he will be called into service and she will go 
back to her mother the house sold. 



g8 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

This couple are quite poor but they have decided to stop worrying about it 
and go to Florida. They have come back from a vacation in Florida, he 
unable to support his wife in a satisfactory manner, joins the army and she 
goes back to her folks. 

Marriage is typically dissolved shortly after its inception, primarily because 
the hero is unable to provide for his wife in a satisfactory manner. The hero 
generally joins the army he is not drafted. At the time of this testing 
the draft was not a serious concern of this subject. 

Now let us consider the application of our theory of repression to the 
analysis of the repressed sexual and aggressive wishes. The sexual wish is 
fused with oral aggression in this record and the consequence of its expression 
can be compared with the consequences of expressing aggression toward the 
father. 

Of approximately 400 stories about 300, or 75 per cent, deal with the 
conflict of either sex or aggression so that this individual is under relatively 
high pressure from both of these wishes. Of these 300 stories, approximately 
100 dealt with aggression and approximately 200 with love and sex. Hence 
we would expect more pressure from the latter conflict than the former, since 
50 per cent of the total pressure of the system would be involved with the 
control of love and sex and 25 per cent with the control of aggression toward 
the father. 

Let us consider next the relative pressure of repressing and repressed 
wishes. In the case of the aggressive wish we rated its pressure as low, since 
it does not appear at all in the face-to-face father-son stones and only gradually 
increases in intensity in the remaining stories. There is clearly less pressure 
to the aggressive wish in this case than in the case of X, who was rated 
medium in pressure, but more pressure than in the case of Y, who was rated 
very low in pressure. Remoteness was rated medium, since there is a gradual 
increase in the expression of aggression with increasing remoteness. It is 
clearly not so remote in expression as in the case of Y, who was rated high, 
and is more remote than in the case of X, who was rated low in remoteness. 
The product of a low pressure repressed wish and a medium remoteness is 
a repressing force of high pressure. There is then a high, low ratio between 
repressing and repressed wish as contrasted with a high, medium ratio for X 
and a high, very low ratio for Y. 

The ratio in the case of the fused love-sex wish resembles the ratio of 
the aggressive wish in the case of X. The pressure of Z's love-sex wish, how- 
ever, is higher than that of his aggression, since it appears in the face-to-face 
situation and has both high intensity and extensity in stories involving love 
objects other than the mother. The conflicts of the hero concerning the control 
of sex are equivalent in pressure to those of X in attempting to control aggres- 



LEVEL ANALYSIS 99 

sion. The love-sex wish is also low in remoteness of expression, appearing 
as a wish to rescue the mother, as an act of aggression toward the sister and 
as undisguised passion towards love objects at one remove from the mother. 
Moreover, the mother loses control of herself while fondling her son. The 
product of the medium pressure of this wish and the low remoteness of 
expression is a high pressure repressing force. This constellation is roughly 
equivalent to the aggression in the case of X. In both cases the individuals 
are chiefly concerned with the control of the wish in one case the aggressive 
wish, in the other the love-sex wish. But in the case of Z's aggression there 
is no indication that any hero ever feels hostile towards the father in face-to-face 
relationships he either runs away or becomes very gentle and effeminate. 
Whenever a hero struggles with the control of a wish we may be certain the 
storyteller is repressing the wish with considerable difficulty and that there 
is a delicate unstable equilibrium between the forces of repression and the 
repressed wish. 

How can we test these derivations? There is a relatively simple method 
which is available. If we could generally weaken the forces of repression we 
would be able to test whether a differential effect was produced by this weaken- 
ing of the forces of repression. We would assume from our estimation of 
forces that some decrease in the forces of repression should allow Z's love-sex 
wish freer expression in face-to-face relations in TAT stories than the aggres- 
sive wish, since the ratio of the repressive force to the love-sex wish is smaller 
than the ratio of repressive force to the aggressive wish. In order to test this 
we administered the TAT to Z while he was under the influence of alcohol. 
It was our prediction that in this state his stories would become frankly 
incestuous in theme, but that the aggression towards the father would not 
appear so openly. Following are two of the stories pertinent to this prediction. 

This poor little dope is looking at a violin. He plays what does he play? 
Sonata on a G String. And all the kids call it on a G string. It must be 
by Rachmaninoff. My mother stood right next to Rachmaninoff. She touched 
him. She went to a concert by him and she stood right beside him. She 
almost swooned. This poor little dope. No doubt he is talcing music lessons. 
Just the way they've got his hair cut into bangs. Still, he is a plump little 
chap. No reason why he doesn't play football. Somewhere back in his mind 
he wants to study, but he doesn't want to be forced to study. The meeting 
having agreed that he doesn't want to be forced to study, we'll say that he 
will be a very good violinist, but not a professional. Maybe he will go into 
engineering as a reaction. Well, the poor little guy has got to be an engineer, 
but music will be very close to his heart. 

This naked woman is the man's sister. I am forced to say that because I 
have said she is his lover for so long that it becomes trite. She is his sister 
and she has had what is considered by the rules of modern society an inappro- 
priate relationship with him. What's that? Incest applies to mother and 



100 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

son as well as sister and brother. A broad term. So much more than pure 
physical love. A soul and a mind. Incest society shudders at the word. 
Why? Why? Society has set up barriers. On a certain island it is indecent 
to be seen eating with a woman. Incest or any other relationship is not 
considered indecent. So here is this woman in love with her brother, her 
brother in love with her. So they have this baby. And this other woman, 
the aunt, is brought up in a Victorian school. She is insulted. She is very 
angry. In her anger she kills the baby. Through projection of her hate for 
her nephew-in-law and her niece, she kills the baby. Life is snuffed out. 
How do perverted relationships differ from normal? 

Our predictions have been confirmed. In the first story the Oedipus 
triangle appears more openly, but the father is cast in the role of Rachmaninoff, 
the master who is capable of making his mother swoon. The sexual meaning 
of this music is indicated by a pun "all the kids call it on a G string." The 
hero in comparison is "this poor little dope" who has had his "hair" cut. 
The hero is clearly envious of the father's virtuosity, and although oppressed 
and inferior, thinks there is really no reason why he could not play a more 
masculine role "No reason why he doesn't play football." He thinks "he 
will be a very good violinist, but not a professional," i.e., he will never be 
able to achieve his father's virtuosity. But then even this is qualified, and 
in the end he "has got to be an engineer, but music will be very close to his 
heart." In other words, though aspiring to the father's place, he does not 
see how he can achieve it. This is a discussion of his feelings towards his 
father, which had never before appeared so openly. But despite this weakening 
of the repressive forces against his aggression there is yet no indication here 
of actual aggression against the father. In the second story the incestuous 
wish towards the sister is given frank, poignant expression. He does not 
understand why it is considered inappropriate. The sister-brother relation- 
ship is thought to be no different than the mother-son relationship "Incest 
applies to mother and son as well as sister and brother. A broad term." He 
is then reminded of the oral nature of his wish "On a certain island it is 
indecent to be seen eating with a woman." But on the same island it is 
thought that incest, if it does not involve oral wishes, is permissible. But 
then conscience in the form of a "Victorian" aunt kills the baby that is the 
fruit of the consummation of their love. The story ends on a pathetic note, 
"How do perverted relationships differ from normal ? " * 

* The affect of this individual while telling these stories was intense and massive. It 
must not be supposed that it was entirely the consequence of alcohol per se. The subject 
had insisted that the examiner drink with him, and this I did. My intoxication matched 
that of the subject's. Previous experience with alcohol administered to subjects in a "scien- 
tific" manner had convinced the examiner that a large part of the reduction of inhibition 
resulting from alcohol intoxication was a consequence of the social atmosphere. An experi- 
menter who dispassionately tests and observes the behavior of an individual who has been 
asked to drink 50 cc. of alcohol will not achieve a reduction of high pressure repressive forces. 



LEVEL ANALYSIS 101 

If so much appears in the TAT, under the influence of alcohol, one may 
ask whether the forces of repression are ever sufficiently weakened to permit 
the expression of overt aggression toward the father. We have found no 
evidence that this ever happens. Dreams over a ten month period revealed 
phantasies very similar to the TAT stories under alcohol. This would be 
consistent with the theory that there is some reduction of vigilance in dreams, 
but that the forces of repression are by no means completely vanquished. 

The following two dreams are typical of those recorded daily for ten 
months. 

I was stripped to the waist when my mother came in. I was glad I hadn't 
put on my shirt Mother didn't seem, to pay much attention to me. I 
embraced my mother. The other woman said "so he didn't go overseas." 
Then I started looking at a pocket sized notebook with a dark green cloth 
cover and black printing hardly noticeable on it. This had been dropped 
beside me by a man who came in with my mother. I then realized it was 
a passport and it was my father's. I looked up and the man was my father 
whom I hadn't recognized till then, 

My mother and I were cleaning floors. We both had mops. First her mop 
got tangled in my hair, which caused very little commotion and then my 
mop got tangled in her hair which upset everyone. I felt very unhappy. 

The first dream is not unlike the first TAT story told under alcohol, 
except that embracing the mother reveals overt expression which was achieved 
only in the second TAT story told under alcohol. The father is recognized 
belatedly and is the apparent cause of the mother's indifference to the son. 
This also repeats the relationship between Rachmaninoff and the son. But 
as in the TAT story there is no aggression toward the father. In the second 
dream the sexual significance of hair appears thinly veiled as it did throughout 
his TAT stories. No one is alarmed by the mother's advances towards the 
son, but everyone, including the hero, is unhappy when he makes advances 
towards the mother. 

The relationship between these dreams, interpreted on the level of manifest 
content, and his TAT stories is of considerable theoretical significance. We 
have seen that his deeply repressed wish to kill his father appears in the TAT 
displaced to remote objects but that it does not appear in his dreams, insofar 
as their manifest content is concerned. This would seem to indicate that 
the censorship which Freud postulated as operating in the dream life is prob- 
ably correct. There would appear to be some relaxation of vigilance, but the 
forces of repression have not been completely inhibited. The TAT seems 
to offer the individual greater distance and more opportunity for the displace- 
ment of deeply repressed wishes to very remote objects. That this type of 
displacement occurs in dreams is also certain but in this case the wish, 



102 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

because o low pressure and higher counterpressure, could not be projected 
into the manifest content of dreams. The latent content of the dream revealed 
through association to elements of the dream stands in the same relationship 
to the manifest content as remote stories to face-to-face normal stories. Thus 
if we regard such a normal story as if it were a dream and ask the indi- 
vidual to free-associate to it we not infrequently arrive at the content of the 
more remote stories. 

In this case we have found further evidence that the return of the 
repressed is a function of the total pressure of the conflict and the relative 
pressure of opposed forces. The sexual conflict was of greater pressure and 
more evenly balanced than that concerning his aggression towards his father. 
For both of these reasons he suffered more anxiety from his incestuous wishes 
than from his aggressive wishes, even though the source of punishment in both 
cases was the father. His stories also indicate that the punishment for incest 
or sexual expression in general is much more severe than the punishment for 
the expression of aggression. The consequence of this difference in strength 
of repression, for his love life and general social relationships, is noteworthy. 
Because the incestuous wish is so close to the surface and countered by a 
relatively high pressure fear there has been a widespread generalization of 
both the wish and the fear to other possible love objects. Because the aggressive 
wish is relatively weaker and under greater counterpressure than in the sexual 
conflict, this wish has not suffered the same degree of generalization to social 
relationships. Z can be more aggressive to other males than he can be tender 
or passionate to other females. 

We have offered these few cases as illustrations rather than as conclusive 
evidence. We do not believe that crucial evidence can be presented until 
these forces can be measured with more precision. We do believe that if the 
techniques of measurement had been more refined and truly quantitative these 
cases could have been offered as crucial evidence for our hypotheses. Despite 
these limitations the methods we have employed have enabled successful 
prediction of the effect of experimental and environmental pressures in several 
other cases in which the TAT indicated high pressure forces in unstable 
equilibrium. 

SUMMARY 

The determination of the relationship between story and storyteller was 
the problem to which this chapter addressed itself. We examined first the rela- 
tionship between overt and covert needs. We maintained that the hypotheses 
offered by Sanford in this connection were probably correct but that the sup- 
porting evidence correlations between needs as expressed in the TAT and 
needs expressed in overt behavior was not so strong as it might have been 
because the TAT did not permit an accurate estimate of the strength of covert 



LEVEL ANALYSIS 103 

needs. This we said was because repressed and suppressed, covert and overt 
needs, the past, present and future are all woven into the imaginative produc- 
tions of die TAT. Since the individual is called upon to interpret the 
behavior, feelings, and expectations of figures represented pictorially, he reveals 
something more than his fleeting diffuse private phantasies. 

The relationship between overt and covert need can frequently be found 
within the TAT stories themselves if attention is given to the levels and 
qualifiers portrayed in the story. Scoring according to the Murray-Sanford 
needs does not differentiate the level of psychological function or specific 
qualifications of such a function. 

We saw that only when a protocol is invariant with respect to the level 
on which the stories proceed may we assume that this level is a literal repre- 
sentation of the predominant level on which that individual functions. An 
example of such invariance is the individual whose anxiety is reflected in his 
stories by exclusive reference to the level of feeling and expectation. Anxiety 
exacerbated by the testing situation itself may produce an invariance of stories 
on the level of object description. Occasionally one finds protocols which 
employ only the behavioral level. Such individuals we have found are extra- 
verted to such an extent that their behavior is imbedded in the external world 
and they are free to a great extent of endopsychic barriers which might inter- 
fere with the spontaneous expression of their wishes. Other individuals may 
be so deeply engrossed in their inner life that instead of assimilating the inner 
life to the external world they assimilate the world to the inner life. If this 
is the case we may find an invariance of the level of feeling or thought or 
memory throughout the protocols. Or the inner life of feeling may assume 
proportions which completely remove the individual from interaction with the 
external world. Stories in such cases employ the level of feeling or mood 
throughout. 

But since most individuals operate on many levels, such protocols as those 
described above are rare. However there are important differences in the 
relative time and energy devoted by different individuals to these levels. A 
very rough index of these differences may be achieved from the relative fre- 
quency of appearance of each level in the protocol. Illustration of the useful- 
ness of such a technique was the analysis of the TAT's of Navaho children 
by Henry, who showed the predominance of behavior and event in their 
protocol and relatively little elaboration of personal feelings, memories and 
daydreams. The TAT's of psychopathic defective criminals were shown by 
Kutash to employ the levels of feeling, description, and wish most frequently, 
which we explained on the basis of their imprisonment and limitation of 
behavior. 

Although frequently useful as a technique of analysis of group data, we 



104 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

said that a much more specific analysis of the different levels was required. 
This involved an analysis of a level that varied which showed that the variation 
was either an effect of variation in some preceding condition, or the cause 
of variation in some consequent condition. 

We next considered the sequence analysis of levels. In this type of 
analysis we disregarded the specific content of the stories and extracted the 
sequence of levels which either preceded or followed any particular level. 
We illustrated in some detail the sequelae and levels which preceded one 
particular level that of wish. The same type of analysis may be applied 
to any other level. Typical sequelae of the level of wish were the following: 
(i) wish-thinking-behavior, signifying a deliberative conjunctive personality 
for whom the expression of any wish necessarily involved an intermediate 
step of thinking; (2) wish-thinking, signifying an overemphasis on the 
deliberative phase of activity, so that action is paralyzed by excessive planning; 
(3) wish-thinking-daydreaming, signifying a short-circuiting of the wish 
through the daydream after some reflection on the wish; (4) wish-daydream- 
behavior, signifying the means-end cognizance to be less important to the 
individual than the vision of the future goal which inspires him to translate 
the wish into appropriate behavior; (5) wish-daydream-expectation, signify- 
ing that the daydream creates sufficient certainty of the future event to require 
no behavior on the part of the hero; (6) wish-daydream, signifying an indi- 
vidual whose daydream is neither preceded by thinking nor followed by 
behavior nor the expectation of a future event, but which simply short-circuits 
the wish; (7) wish-daydream-event, signifying an individual who has in fact 
received an abundance of gratuities without effort on his part or a wish- 
fulfillment phantasy; (8) wish-event, signifying that the individual is the 
passive object of external forces (the event may gratify or frustrate the satis- 
faction of the wish) ; (9) wish-behavior is a curiously rare sequence, probably 
because the extraverted individual who is imbedded in the external world is 
less aware of the existence of wishes as separate entities; (10) wish followed by 
no other level commonly signifies an individual whose wishes by their very 
nature seem impossible of gratification; (n) wish-counterwish-behavior sig- 
nifies either a somewhat inhibited or impulsive personality, depending on 
which wish is expressed in behavior. We next considered the levels which 
preceded the level of wish: (i) event-wish signifies an individual whose inner 
life is essentially reactive to outer stimuli of an impersonal nature; (2) memory- 
wish commonly signifies an individual who suffers grief, guilt, shame, or 
anxiety since one of the salient characteristics of such inner states is their 
peculiar sensitivity to rearousal through memory; (3) physical sensations-wish 
signify an individual governed to a great extent by bodily states (this is par- 
ticularly characteristic of children and adolescents); (4) expectation-wish signi- 



LEVEL ANALYSIS 



105 



fies an individual whose inner life is governed either by his own expectations 
or those of others; (5) special state- wish signifies that the wish is under some 
inhibition in the normal state and some de-inhibiting force is required to 
activate it; (6) perception- wish signifies, in the event that this is the perception 
of human objects, an individual whose inner life is governed by the presence 
of other human beings; (7) behavior-wish, if this is the behavior of others, 
signifies an individual whose wishes are essentially reactive to the behavior 
of others; (8) moods- or feelings-wish signify an individual whose inner life 
exerts massive pressure and is more self regulative than reactive to outer stimuli. 

We also examined a single story to illustrate the more complex sequence 
of levels usually found in the protocol. The particular sequence was behavior- 
event-thinking-mood-wish. We saw that these sequences of levels cannot be 
assumed literally to represent something typical of the storyteller unless that 
sequence is invariant in the protocol. If it is not invariant we must examine 
the conditions under which the sequences vary to determine the meaning of 
the variability. 

We next examined the problem of awareness the degree to which the 
individual is aware of his own wishes or behavior. We saw that awareness 
of the nature of overt behavior might be limited to the awareness of single 
concrete fragments of behavior that the patterning of behavioral sequences 
might entirely escape the attention of the individual that the contemporary 
determinants of such patterns were even more elusive, and that the relationship 
of these to their genetic precursors was the achievement of only the most 
reflective individuals. We said that an individual may possess awareness of 
his own motivation and not attribute it to his heroes but that we had not 
found individuals who attributed more awareness of motivation to their 
heroes than they themselves possessed of their own motivation. One typically 
finds in the TAT stories of patients who have received psychotherapy attribu- 
tion of complex patterns of motivation to their heroes, but psychotherapy is 
not the only road to such insight. We presented 6 "insightful" stories, 3 from 
individuals who had been psychoanalyzed and 3 whose stories were told with- 
out benefit of prior therapeutic intervention. 

We examined next the problem of repression. We said that discrepancies 
between what is known of the individual's daily behavior and the behavior 
of his heroes provides a simple criterion for distinguishing whether the latter 
represents overt behavior or covert wish. The problem of assessing levels of 
psychological function within the covert sphere was seen to be much more 
complex and difficult. Although forces within the covert sphere may exert 
varying degrees of pressure for admittance into consciousness or translation 
into behavior, behavioral criteria for the measurement of such differences in 
pressure have not yet been clearly defined. 



I06 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

In order to exploit the sensitivity of the TAT in eliciting repressed material, 
we undertook a revision and extension of Freud's theory of repression. 

We assumed first that any personality has a finite quantum of energy at its 
disposal. Secondly, a personality may be treated for analytical purposes as if 
it were a closed system. We defined psychological energy in terms of units 
of psychological pressure and assumed that any single wish within the per- 
sonality may have any pressure not exceeding the total amount of pressure 
available to the personality. We also assumed that, because of the finite 
quantum of pressure units available to the person, the expenditure of pressure 
units in one area limits the amount of pressure available for other purposes. 
We further assumed that the unit of pressure is the product of two components 
intensity and extensity. By intensity we meant the strength of the wish or 
drive and by extensity its scope or mass. Thus the pressure of a wish might 
be equivalent if it were either of high intensity and low extensity or low 
intensity and high extensity. 

It followed from these assumptions that as the total pressure of two wishes 
in conflict increases the pressure available for the remainder of the system 
decreases. Thus a conflict between two wishes of low pressure rarely involves 
pathogenic sequelae, but a conflict between two wishes of high pressure is the 
condition par excellence for pathology, since if each wish involved almost 
half of the total pressure of the personality this would be civil war within 
the individual. 

Whether a conflict will be pathogenic, and more specifically, whether the 
repressed will "return" are also functions of the relative pressure of the two 
forces which stand in opposition to each other. It was our hypothesis that 
as the repressing force increases in pressure, relative to the repressed force, 
the possibility of return of the repressed wish decreases and, further, that this 
entails a reduction in disturbance to the personality as a whole. Conversely, 
we assumed that as the balance between the two forces in opposition approached 
equality there would be a greater tendency of the repressed wish to return 
and an increase in general disturbance to the personality as a whole. Our 
doctrine was in this respect contrary to that contemporary opinion which 
attributes serious pathogenic potentialities to "deeply" repressed material. We 
maintained in effect that the deeper the repression the less pathogenic the 
conflict. 

If these hypotheses are correct, the most serious conflict would be one 
in which there were two fairly evenly balanced forces, consuming between them 
the entire energies of the system. The least serious conflict would be that 
between two forces of low pressure but in which one of the forces was rela- 
tively much higher in pressure than the other, although between them a 
relatively small part of the total pressure of the system was involved. As a 



LEVEL ANALYSIS 107 

conflict approached either of these end points it would be more or less patho- 
genic. It would also follow that if one factor was favorable the pathogenic 
potentiality of the other factor would be reduced. 

We next considered the empirical problem of the measurement of these 
forces. We saw that the measurement of the absolute and relative strength 
of two forces, one of which represses the other, is peculiarly difficult because 
the nature of the repressed force can be investigated only when the pressure 
of the repressing force has been reduced to zero. It was our suggestion that 
the analysis of the individual's imaginative productions did allow a more 
satisfactory measure of these forces than we could at present achieve through 
experimental investigation. We thought this was so because the forces of 
repression were greatly reduced by allowing the individual to achieve distance 
between himself and the characters of his stories and by the use of pictures 
which are either ambiguous, bizarre, or remote. Such pictures permit the 
individual license in telling stories unfettered by realistic considerations. 
Further, he is encouraged to do this by the change of instructions at the 
beginning of the second session that he may give freer rein to his imagina- 
tion. For these reasons the individual may attribute wishes to his heroes 
which no amount of alcohol or other experimental manipulation might induce 
him to express in overt behavior. But since the force of repression is only 
weakened and not reduced to a zero quantity, repressed wishes are usually 
not given completely free expression even under these conditions. We must 
therefore employ the technique of comparing the behavior of those heroes who 
are interacting "normally" with parents or others with those heroes who are 
represented in a setting psychologically "remote" from normal interaction. It 
was our assumption that the remoteness of conditions under which antisocial 
wishes might be expressed was a function of the relative pressure of repressing 
and repressed forces. As the repressing force increases in pressure relative to 
the pressure of the repressed force, the conditions under which the latter may 
be given expression in TAT stories become more and more "remote." As this 
ratio approaches equality the expression of the repressed force will appear 
under less and less remote conditions in the stories. 

We suggested that the ratio of the combined pressure of repressing and 
repressed wish to the total pressure might be crudely estimated by totaling 
the number of stories which refer either to the repressing force or the repressed 
force and computing the percentage of such stories to the total number of 
stories. In order to determine the pressure of the repressed wish we must 
rate the intensity and extensity of the repressed wish as it appears in each 
story estimate the product of both components and then add these separate 
estimates, to achieve a total estimate of the pressure of the repressed wish. 
In order to estimate the pressure of the repressing force we would employ 



I08 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

our hypothesis that remoteness is a function of the ratio of repressing pressure 
to repressed pressure. Having estimated the pressure of the repressed wish 
and the degree of remoteness, the product of these two estimates provides 
an estimate of the pressure of the repressing force. 

We then presented three cases, X, Y, and Z, to illustrate these hypotheses. 
Each of the TAT's revealed that aggression against a parent had been repressed 
by these individuals. In the case of X, this wish and its control expended 
much of the available energy, representing a high pressure-medium pressure 
conflict with low remoteness of expression of aggression. In the case of Y 
the combined pressure of repressing force and aggression was high. The 
repressing force was of high pressure but the aggression was of very low 
pressure, having high intensity but very low extensity. Remoteness of expres- 
sion of aggression was high. In the case of Z this conflict consumed less energy 
than with X or Y. It represented a high pressure-low pressure conflict. 
Remoteness of expression of aggression represented a mid-point between the 
other cases, being of medium degree. In accordance with our hypotheses, 
X was the most seriously disturbed, Y least disturbed, and Z represented 
a mid-point between these two. We further showed that a special hypothesis 
in the case of Z was confirmed by experimental evidence. The incestuous 
wish, we said, was opposed by a relatively weaker force than was the aggres- 
sive wish and should therefore appear first if the forces of repression were 
somewhat weakened. This prediction was confirmed by the stories which 
he told under the de-inhibiting effect of alcohol. 



CHAPTER VI 

DIAGNOSIS OF PERSONALITY: THE REGION OF 
THE FAMILY 

Freud's dictum of the importance of the family romance pervades modern 
psychopathology and personality theory and has so insinuated itself into the 
contemporary climate of opinion that only today, partly as a result of the war 
experience, are we beginning to question whether a halt should be called 
to the now almost unconscious assumption of strict causal relationship between 
early childhood and adult personality. The TAT provides a valuable technique 
for assaying, with more precision than we could before, the exact relationships 
obtaining between the personality of the child as it is formed in the family 
setting and the adult personality. Our purpose in this chapter, however, is 
twofold: we shall delineate techniques useful in such assessment and more 
generally address ourselves to the criteria for determining the relative impor- 
tance of the family region for the individual personality. 

*. DIRECT REFERENCES 

The protocols may contain direct references to the importance of the 
family to the individual. 

This is a picture of a mother and son. The father died when the boy was 
only two and so his mother has been more than a mother. She was both 
mother and father, and they have always been very close to each other. She 
means more to him than anything else in the world, and now when he has to 
leave her to go into the army, he doesn't know how to tell her. But she 
senses it, and is understanding about it as she has always been about every- 
thing. When he comes back from the army she will still be there, and they 
will go on as if nothing has ever happened. 

B. INTRODUCTION OF PARENTAL FIGURES 

Such direct references are not common. In general more indirect tech- 
niques must be employed. One such technique involves an examination of 
the protocol for the frequency of introduction of parental figures, where the 
picture contains none. Such introduction must however be evaluated further 
in terms of the specific picture. Thus in picture i, the boy with the violin, 
the introduction of a parent is less significant than a similar introduction 
in a story to the blank card. The possibilities of introducing people or objects 
into the blank card are endless but the possibilities of such introduction are 

109 



110 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

limited in the former by the fact that the parent is the usual mediator of the 
child's musical training in the American family. The significance of such 
introduction, however, must not be overlooked simply because it reflects 
the cultural setting. Common cultural influences are no less important than 
individual differences to an understanding of personality. 

C. AMBIGUOUS FIGURES INTERPRETED AS PARENTAL FIGURES 

Another criterion is the interpretation of ambiguous figures as parents. 
Such pictures as 3BM (the figure huddled against a couch), 10 (two vague 
figures embracing), 14 (a silhouette of a figure), and 20 (an ambiguous figure 
standing under a lamp) may be seen as individuals of any age or sex, and 
when the family is more important than other regions these individuals may 
be seen as parents. 

D. OLDER FIGURES INTERPRETED AS PARENTAL FIGURES 

For children, almost any figure that is not a child may be seen as a parent, 
whereas for early and late adolescents and adults the picture of an older adult 
presents the option of interpretation as either an older adult or a parent. With 
the qualifications treated in the discussion of repression, there is a direct 
relationship between the importance of the parent in the life of the storyteller 
and the number of such figures interpreted as parents rather than other adults. 
This relationship, however, only exists when identification is with the child 
in the story. An adult may identify with the parents in his stories and in 
doing this is telling us that the role of parent has superseded his earlier role 
as a child. 

E. OMISSION OF OLDER ADULTS IN THE PICTURE 

If older adults in the picture are omitted in the elaboration of a story 
it may signify either that parents and parental surrogates have ceased to be 
important or that the individual denies their existence because for some reason 
they are unwanted, dangerous, or otherwise unacceptable to him, 

F. NUMBER OF STORIES ABOUT THE FAMILY 

A further index of the importance of the family is the proportion of 
stories concerned with the family rather than with love, work, or social 
relationships. 

G. LENGTH OF STORIES ABOUT THE FAMILY 

The length of stories told about the family in comparison with the length 
of other stories also provides a useful criterion. 



THE REGION OF THE FAMILY III 

H. INTENSITY OF AFFECT IN STORIES ABOUT THE FAMILY 

An individual sometimes tells only one story concerning the family, but 
the affect of the storyteller and the affect attributed to the hero in the story 
are in striking contrast to every other story told by him. In such cases the 
individual may be attempting to deny or forget his own family and this is a 
single instance of the break-through of the feeling he is trying to control. 
However, if every story, whatever the theme, is characterized by intense affect 
this is a less diagnostic criterion of the relative significance of the family. 

I. CONFLICT WITH OTHER REGIONS 

The importance of the family may be such that it eliminates all possi- 
bility of conflict with other regions, or the family may have become so 
unimportant that no conflict exists. But if the individual is neither completely 
immersed in nor uninterested in the family, the intensity of conflict which he 
mentions between the family and other regions and the resolution of this 
conflict provides an index of the relative importance of the family and other 
regions. We are not referring primarily to stories of direct parental interven- 
tion in other regions, a topic which will be considered later, but to stories 
involving the hero's own conflict about the relative valences of the family 
and other regions. Such conflict, or the lack of it, is commonly expressed 
in response to picture 2, showing an older man and woman and a young girl. 

In the following stories we shall illustrate the presence or absence of 
conflict, the oscillations that may occur, and the degrees of residual tension 
if the conflict is resolved either in favor of the family or another region. 

As we have said, the hero may have a minimum interest in or conflict with 
anything outside the family. 

This picture gives a sense of the closeness of a family to the soil. They are 
all tied together in a common undertaking. The soil is fertile and these 
people have almost a religious feeling about their home. The mother's face 
denotes calmness and serenity indicative of her contentment. Her husband 
is bending his strength toward plowing, and although his .face is not visible, 
the degree of physical perfection which is his is shown by the musculature of 
his back and shoulders outlined by his toil. The daughter is obviously a 
student She has a dreamy look in her eyes, dreaming of the summertime 
when school is out and she can spend all her time with her mother and father 
on the farm. 

Or the heroine may, as in the following two stories, be interested in other 
values and leave the family, only to discover that the parental values are really 
her values; whereupon she returns to the family, experiencing no residual 
tension after her return. 



112 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

This girl was brought up on a farm. The man and the woman are her 
mother and father, and she has just returned from the school, and she is 
looking over the land and wondering if it is worth all the energy and time 
to cultivate the land as they do. Later on she'll ask her mother and father 
for permission to go to the city to live with her aunt. When she gets to the 
city she enrolls in a very swanky girls' school. There she meets girls from 
different social classes than hers. To those girls the only important things are 
clothes and money, and she didn't like this new life. After a month of this 
kind of life she decides to go home. She now realizes that her mother and 
father's work that her mother and father's work is necessary, and she will 
devote her life to helping them. 

A good old farm scene. The son has worked hard all his life on the farm 
and the father has worked beside him. The mother has slaved day in and 
day out with all the work of the house and feeding all the farm hands. They 
have gotten along all right in life without any real education. But what's 
this the daughter of the family is carrying her beloved books around with 
her. She doesn't want to stay on the farm all the rest of her life. She reads 
every chance she gets. Why should she be stuck away out here she wanted 
to go to the city and get somewhere. But her parents just laughed at her. 
What was good enough for them, was all right for her. And so she lived 
day and night snatching a couple of minutes when she wasn't doing some 
chore to read a little bit. She read at night when all around her was silent. 
Their farm was prosperous, but she never got away until one day she finally 
made up her mind to get out. She didn't tell her folks, but left one night 
when they were all sleeping. After being in the city for a long time she found 
out how stupid she had been to leave her family, home, and friends. So she 
came back meekly and she only then appreciated how marvelous her farm 
life was. 

Or the heroine may intensely dislike home but stay, suppressing the wish 
to leave home. 

This woman is trying to talk some sense into this girl. She is reading a 
preceptual passage to her but the girl's mind is far away. She does not want 
to listen to this woman her mother because she associates most of her 
previous humiliating experiences with her. She wants to get away, to be 
free. She has mixed feelings of self-pity, aggression, and a desire to start out 
on her own. However she makes an effort and suppresses this mood and 
listens to what her mother is reading. 

Or the individual, although she rejects the parents and is interested in 
other values, may have no way of escaping them and remains at home, suffering 
much residual tension. 

On a farm on a lake in Michigan lived a family who had had to toil hard 
to wrest a living from the unwilling soil. But in spite of their hardships and 
sufferings caused by the soil and its obstinate ways, they had all gotten to 
love it and were content to spend their lives working their sometimes beloved, 
sometimes hated, but always their own soil. Mother had come there as a 



THE REGION OF THE FAMILY 113 

young bride and there had had and brought up all her children, and Joe, the 
eldest son who was taking father's place, had really made the farm advance 
and was quite content to work there. But it is not right to say that all 
loved it there leading that monotonous life with no chance for a higher 
development, for Martha, die daughter, hated it. She was sick of depending 
solely on the soil for an existence and being among people content to live there 
and never get anywhere in life. She wanted to learn all she could and get 
out in the world, away from the farm and make a place for herself in the 
world that was really so fascinating if you only looked beyond the full same- 
ness of the farm. But was there any plausible hope for getting away? No 
only that of staying on the farm and getting some knowledge out of what 
books she could obtain. And she was right for Martha never managed to 
get away. Though she did read many books and became more worldly-wise 
than the others of her family, in time she too was sucked into the soil, the 
farm, and the life there, as if by quicksilver. 

But if the family values are unsatisfying the outside world may present 
different problems; the individual, unable to leave or to stay, may not resolve 
the conflict but live the kind of "between two worlds" existence described in 
the next story. 

Ah a young woman in the country, a young girl rather, who is somewhat 
above average intelligence and some sensibility, who doesn't like, who didn't 
care for her family and for the life in the country and whose ambition was to 
get to college, and there was a little mountain college in the state, in her 
end of the state, and after much thrashing about she finally managed to, ah, 
get to the college but found that it, it didn't suit her either, or rather that she 
didn't 'suit it. She couldn't quite do the work and that people, ah, didn't, 
didn't care for her and so forth. She would come home at times she would 
go to college and be homesick and then get on a bus or something and come 
home and so in the mountainous part of the state and come home. Pre- 
sumably when she was at home she would be miserable because she had not 
cared for her home in the first place. Ah, in die picture here she is [laughs] 
well, just as well, let's say just leaving, just going back on the way to college 
again, leaving her mother and perhaps mother and older sister at the farm. 
It's a kind of between-two-worlds thing where neither world is at all excellent, 
has no virtue in itself, but where one is not enough and the other is too much 
for this ah, simple person. 

Or the individual seeking other values may leave home but suffer a great deal 
of residual tension. 

This girl lives all by herself. She comes home from work and retires to her 
lonely room. She sits in a chair and starts to ponder over what her life has 
been. She thinks of her childhood at home with her parents and of the 
many opportunities that were open to her. She thinks of her own stubbornness 
and selfishness. She wonders why she didn't take advantage of the oppor- 
tunities. As a child she -was given many things that other children did not 
have. When she grew older, a she decided that she wanted to leave home 



114 THB THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

and be by herself. She left home and went to a big city. There she got a 
job, thinking she'd be able to meet the kind of people she always wanted to 
know. But she finds herself one of many thousands of lonely girls with no 
close friends and no family to turn to. 

Or throwing off parental standards, the individual may leave home and 
experience a minimum of residual tension. Rarely do we find an individual 
who has absolutely no attachment to or interest in the family, but that this 
interest may be slight and other values more important is seen in this story. 

This reminds me of a book. I can't remember the name of the book, but it 
was about a swamp and a girl. I think that the girl is seeing beyond the 
farm. That she is in deep thought. She was born with industrious, hard 
working parents. As she grew up, she saw all the toil of the farm. She has 
a desire to step out from the bounds of the farm. The book she is carrying 
bring this out, and the fact that her back is facing the farm. She doesn't hate 
it, but she wants to do greater things. Her parents are bound up with 
work, and her parents don't resent her going off. She is going to high school 
and is going through much trouble because of her queer appearance, but she 
has the strength of character that will make her successful. She will be 
successful. She has a hard time making friends. She will marry an eminent 
person the professor type, and she will be very happy. She's not content 
to watch the world go by. She will be eminent in some field either she's 
interested in painting or something along those lines be famous. She'll be 
happier than if she were on the farm. She has character and foresight that 
her parents lack. No person to lead a humdrum life. She's not afraid of 
making rash statements, but will do what she thinks is right. 

J. RANGE OF PARENTAL IMPACT WITHIN THE FAMILY SETTING 

Another criterion of the relative importance of the family is the degree 
of parental impact within the family setting. By degree of parental impact 
we mean the extent to which the child's personality is actually shaped by the 
parent. Not infrequently the influence of the parent may be so pervasive that 
the child literally has no area of free movement. Whether the child identifies 
completely with the parent, is submissive to parental dominance, or rebels 
against the parent, his personality is the resultant of massive parental impact. 
At the opposite end of the continuum are parents who, either through indif- 
ference or complete submission to the child's demands, exert relatively little 
influence on the formation of his personality. 

Parental impact is a complex resultant of parent-child interaction. There 
may, for example, be litde influence on the child because the parent is either 
indifferent or submissive to the child, or if he attempts to govern or influence 
the child his influence may still be slight because of the child's indifference; 
or in the face of the child's resistance to any attempts to govern or influence 



THE REGION OF THE FAMILY 115 

him, the parent may retreat, abandoning any idea of trying to influence him 
or in some cases even becoming submissive to the child. 

On the other hand, the parent may have great influence whether the child 
is submissive or rebellious to attempts to govern or influence him. In the case 
of the child who rebels the influence may still be great because his personality 
becomes the exact opposite of the parental ideal. The influence may also be 
great if the child identifies with the parent, whether or not the parent is instru- 
mental in fostering this identification. 

Between these extremes of maximal and minimal parental impact are 
many varying degrees of influence. The parent may exert some influence on 
the child indirectly by exposing him to other influences; parental ego idealism 
may extract half-hearted compliance from the child, or the parents may influ- 
ence the child only on special occasions under special circumstances or for a 
certain length of time. 

In the following stories we present a sample, ranging from a minimum 
to a maximum of parental impact. In the first story parental impact is slight; 
the child wishes the parents to do more for him than they're capable of doing 
and their lack is the stimulus specific to his development of independence and 
initiative: 

Well, it looks as though the violin is broken and he feels sad about it because 
he likes to play. Uh . . . he's about eight years old and he wants to be a 
violinist. His family can't afford to get him another one. He's probably 
thinking about how he can earn some money to get it repaired. He gets a 
job delivering newspapers and gets the violin repaired and takes up his studies 
again. Because of his interest he becomes a success. 

In the next story parental impact is still very limited. The parents are 
capable of doing what the child wishes but the child is the instigator of their 
activity and they merely minister to his needs. 

Sitting there is a boy about 10 years old gazing at a violin. Ah he's quite an 
intelligent fellow and ah I think perhaps he wanted to study the violin. So 
his mother and father bought him the violin, and he started to take lessons. 
As the lessons progressed, he found there was more work involved than he 
had thought. He realizes that success wouldn't come as easily as he thought, 
and he will have to study and study before he becomes a great violinist. By 
the expression in his eyes, it looks as if he's wondering whether it's worth 
all the effort As he grows older he stops taking violin lessons and listens 
to concerts. He will realize he was wrong to stop taking violin lessons. 

The role of the parents in the following story is also limited; they minister 
to the child's needs as in the preceding story but they play a part in shaping 
the child's interest, even though indirectly, by .exposing him to other influences. 



13 6 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

Once when Frederick was a very, very, very, little boy his father took him 
along to hear a famous violinist play at the Opera House. During the 
performance and on the way home, Frederick had said nothing nor did he 
for days afterwards. Instead he had a dreamy look of yearning in his eyes 
and he went around humming the pieces played by the violinist. Finally 
one afternoon his father, in his study, looked up to find the huge door being 
slowly pushed open by little Frederick. The boy came over to him and, with 
the same look of yearning in his eyes, asked if he could please, -please have a 
violin of his own; that to play a violin was all he wanted ever in the wide 
world. His father didn't say much, but soon a big box came for Frederick. 
Hardly daring to hope, he opened it slowly, and lo there it was his own 
violin. He took it out gingerly and lovingly and set it on the table before him 
and just looked at it with that same loving, longing look. And do you know 
who that little Frederick is now? After studying and studying and studying, 
he became one of our famous violinists. 

The parent in the next story attempts to play an active role in influencing 
the child, but the child's resistance induces the parent to conform to his wishes 
and the impact is limited. 

I'll try to make them different. This one is hard to vary. He's obviously 
contemplating his violin with disgust. He's either disgusted or sleepy. Both 
indicate boredom at best. The violin was given him by his idealistic mother 
who thought it would be fine if Junior could become a virtuoso with the 
violin. She thought that her child ought to become artistically accomplished 
in one way or the other. Since her father used to play the fiddle in some 
church gatherings in the old days she thought it would be nice if Junior 
could maintain or at least establish a family tradition. The rub is that Junior 
doesn't go for the idea at all! This represents his attitude at the end of one 
week of playing and he is pretty fed up. His mother, who is also a sensible 
person besides being an idealist, decides that there's no point in her forcing 
him to do something that he is not willing to do, so she gives the violin to 
the U.S.O., and he is afterward unencumbered by the necessity of doing a 
slavish routine. 

In the following story, the parents, having attempted to play an active 
role, retire in the face of the child's resistance as in the preceding story but 
continue to exert influence indirectly, inspiring the child by exposing him to 
other influences. 

Well, some time ago this little fellow, his friend wanted, who is studying the 
piano and he thought he had heard of a violin so he thought he wanted to 
study the violin. He didn't realize the amount of time it would demand 
of him, but then anyway after he had studied several months he decided an 
hour a day was taking too much of his time and he decided after that, from 
then on he decided that he would rebel, revolt against the idea of practicing 
every day. This is, oh, a picture represents one afternoon about two and a 
half months after he had started his violin practice. He is very disgusted; 
this afternoon he doesn't want to practice his violin. His mother has been 



THE REGION OF THE FAMILY 1 17 

coaxing him but without much success and he is just as stubborn and per- 
sistent as his mother. He is sitting there and in a kind of a fit after the 
afternoon since he won't practice his mother has decided that, well, she is 
going to have to approach him in a little different way so she bargains with 
him that he can go out this afternoon on condition mat he will practice his 
hour tomorrow without any trouble. That seems to be O.K. with him. It's 
a struggle when he comes down to practicing tomorrow but he carries off 
his part of the bargain. Ah, since he is a little fellow he is a, his mother 
has decided that her whole approach to music should be different. He 
shouldn't be forced to practice. He should be made to want to practice and 
so starting from this moment on she takes him to concerts and she gets, oh, 
small muscial biographies with which she tries to inspire him. Strangely 
enough, this is very effective and from that moment on he aspires, he starts 
to enjoy his practice. He aspires to greater things, undoubtedly the concert 
stage. Later on in his teens he is sent to a conservatory to study. He has 
become a, oh, a bit of an unusual player or he would never have been sent 
to the conservatory, but he decides to make music and the violin particularly 
his profession. Along in the past it became his, his life ambition to play on 
the concert stage. And of course, from his two-hour, two- and three-hour 
practice a day at home it is quite a jump in the conservatory where he has to 
devote seven to ten hours a day to his music, studying harmony, particularly 
the violin along with piano. Of course, after many struggles . comes the big 
night when as, ah, the prize student of the conservatory he is asked to play 
with the shall we say the Boston Symphony Orchestra? some famous concerto, 
Tschaikowsky's B Flat Concerto, and of course as an ordinary young genius 
he meets with success but it's just a sign of the success he will meet later 
as a great, as a great violinist: 

In the preceding two stories it was the parent who realized the inadvisa- 
bility of forcing her ideals on the child, but in the next story the child having 
conformed for three years suddenly rejects the parental dictate without waiting 
for parental sanction. 

Little Jimmy is a normal little boy about eight years old. He is a member of 
the neighborhood gang and loves nothing better than to go out and hunt 
snakes, tadpoles and all sorts of insects. Besides that he is second base on the 
gang baseball team and now that it is four-thirty his mother calls him in to 
practice his violin. He has had to play the violin for three years and all the 
fellows razz him because he has to go in daily at four-thirty to practice and 
in his mind Jimmy can't understand why his parents make him play the 
violin when he is planning on some day being a fireman, and he doesn't 
plan on sitting around playing the fiddle like some emperor did. As he sits 
brooding over the violin Jimmy decides right then and there that he will not 
practice any longer and that this is all useless. 

Dominance stemming from the parent's ideal for the child governs the 
behavior of the child through adolescence in the following story, but in early 
adulthood the child rejects the ideal and though the parent is dissatisfied he 
yields. 



Il8 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

This little boy whose father was a musician forced his younger child into 
playing the violin. Somehow he had no desire to play, in spite of his father, 
and being forced into playing was torture for him. Day after day he 
would sit in front of his violin which lay on the table and with much 
discouragement, and hate would pick it up at intervals and try again but 
without success. This same performance went on for several years. At the 
age of eighteen he still had made no progress as his father had wished he 
would, and so he left his violin and took up the law which was what he 
wanted to do ever since he was a youngster. He was successful at law, and 
his father was pleased, but he was much dissatisfied that his son never became 
a musician like himself. 

Although the child in the next story wishes to do something else and 
parental idealism extracts only half-hearted compliance and passive resistance, 
he does not completely reject the parental dictate as in the preceding two stories. 
Thus the parent exerts somewhat more influence. 

After two years of daily practice, Billy still did not like to play the strings of 
his violin. But his father being a celebrated violinist in a symphony orchestra 
had one desire that this son should one day become a famous musician like 
himself. He instructed Billy twice a week, mostly in vain, because many a 
day Billy would just sit for hours looking at the wooden structure with 
strings on it. Strings, he thought, that probably came from a horse's tail. 
A horse that had probably broken his leg and had to be put away. Now that's 
what Billy wanted to do. He wanted to be a veterinarian and care for all 
animals. 

The ideal of the parents in the following story is also foreign to the child's 
wishes but he conforms because he does not want to disappoint them. Parental 
impact is great and when he reaches maturity conformance with parental 
wishes has been sufficiently rewarding to bring about complete identification 
with parental values. 

Many years ago a boy got a present for his birthday. The reason that they 
gave him a violin was because they wanted him to become as interested in 
music as they were. As the little boy sat looking at the beautiful instrument, 
disappointment showed in his face. He thought how much nicer it would have 
been if his parents had given him some toys. He realized how much the 
present meant to them so he tried to look as happy as possible. He was 
curious about the violin because he had never seen one before; he had only 
heard his father and mother talk about them, so he decided to see what it 
would do. He picked up the bow and drew it across the strings. He liked 
the sound and tried it again. Wouldn't it be fun to put all the sounds 
together, he thought. Now standing off stage at ^Carnegie Hall, waiting for 
the applause to die down, this great artist thanks his parents from the 
bottom of his heart. 

But 2, as in the f ollowing story, the child loves the parent deeply, parental 
ego idealism may hold him to a course in spite of occasional moments of resent- 



THE REGION OF THE FAMILY 119 

ment. In such a case the impact on the development of the child and on the 
resultant adult personality is massive. 

Why must I practice so hard? and so long? and every day? And yet I love 
mother so. I must make a success of this violin playing. Oh I must! 
Mother is counting on it. It's everything to her; if 'I fail and can't become 
very famous as ... well the Great Jonny Jerome or Mrs. Albert Jerome's 
Jonny Jerome . . . that would be better still, I certainly will deserve the 
booby prize. Then mother can dress up and feel very proud and forget her 
failure. Mother, gosh she must have been very pretty. Somehow they didn't 
see how talented she was ... or maybe her voice was too small ... it sounds so 
pretty here in the apartment but maybe it didn't sound like that in the big 
opera houses; maybe that's why she wasn't a success. I've got to work hard, 
real hard, terribly hard so she will be pleased. Tommy and Bud just don't 
understand why I'm so gone on this fiddle and catgut. It's motherwhen 
some extra special sound comes out she is so happy. Gee. I want an 
awful lot of extra special sounds to come 'cause she seems to even have Dad 
with her in this room here on earth, not really but just pretend. . . . Now, 
Jonny Jerome, are you going to sleep when you have another hour to practice? 
Here goes . . . turn ... tee turn. The following is an excerpt from a local 
newspaper printed some 10 years later. "A young, blond and handsome chap, 
known to this town's other young and handsome chaps as 'jonny* and to the 
'olsters' as Mrs. Albert Jerome's son who plays the violin so well has been 
claimed by both 'chaps' and 'olsters' as the GREAT JEROME. Last evening 
he strummed and plucked those strings as no other seventeen year old since 
Paganini has ever dared. If we are not mistaken we see a brilliant future 
for this violinist. Mrs. Albert Jerome may well be proud of such a son." 

I. SCOPE OF PARENTAL INTERVENTION IN OTHER REGIONS 

The degree of parental influence is to some extent a function of the scope 
of such parental impact in regions outside the family. The parent may influ- 
ence everything the child does inside or outside of the family, as in the 
following story. 

The girl has been so watched by her parents that she feels herself going crazy. 
She has to tell them to the minute where she goes and what she does. Then 
her mind collapses and she spends years in an institution before she becomes 
normal again. 

Every crucial region may be influenced by the parents but with less rigid 
control than in the pre9eding story. In the stories which follow, told by an 
eighteen year old girl, we see that work, love, and social relationships are all, 
to a great extent, under the influence of the parents. 

What was a mother for but to comfort her child when in distress? If only 
she could think of something to say or do that would help her daughter. 
Mary had come to her in tears and sobs that shook her whole body. And no 
wonder Bob had been such a rat about the whole thing. Ah, poor dear 



120 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

Mary. Come, lean on me and sob it out. Yes, I know, it was ten times more 
awful to learn it from another person, and especially that silly mean Gertie. 
It would have been better if Bob himself had told you but here's one consolation 
now you know what kind of a person he is, so you can free yourself of 
him in time. And, Mary dear, you're young still, really very young, with 
the largest part of your life still before you. You can be happy you know 
it is possible in spite of this part of your life. We can go away for a while 
and forget Bob and Gertie and let them do what they please. There are other 
men in the world. When we come back they will probably have left, and 
you can do some work and it will all turn out all right in time really it will. 
Jane and her mother often sat together, Jane playing with her doll and her 
mother reading to her. But today was a little different. Jane couldn't sit 
back and comfortably become enthralled with the doll or the story, for she 
was waiting to go to a party. They lived out in the country and it was a 
rainy, bad day with the roads consequently turned into mires, so she knew 
it was likely her hostess' chauffeur would be later in coming for her. However, 
she also knew in her heart, but didn't like to admit it to herself, that it 
was very doubtful whether he could get through at all. Her mother was 
being very sweet and reading a lovely story to take her mind off that possibility, 
but still, she didn't want a story now, she wanted to go to a party. When 
Jane had just about given up all hope of going, she heard the sound of a 
car coming slowly and painfully down the drive. So she could go to the 
party after all! Her mother heard it too and put down the book and with a 
smile got Jane's hat and coat. They were ready and waiting when the car 
got to the door and Jane drove off at last to the party waving back to her 
mother with the happiest face. 

What else could he have done gone away and just never admitted it to her, 
in the hope she would just forget he ever existed? His own mother. Never! 
Being his own mother it probably would have hurt him more to desert her than 
to do as he had done and come back to tell her everything and let her know 
that she had brought her son up to be a total failure. But the look on her face 
when he had appeared. He'll never forget that! Of course she had known the 
minute he came in for why would he have returned if he hadn't failed? She 
hadn't said anything, just turned away with a sad look that said "So, he's 
failed, which means I've failed, and now what is there left now?" Practically 
nothing. He could hardly bring himself to think of it. He had lost all hi? 
father's money the money that his father and mother, and then just hi? 
mother, had scraped together and saved penny by penny, the money to start 
him out in this business. And now all their hopes and dreams as well as 
work, bound up in that money which would send their son on his way to 
success, were all gone for nothing. But he could still fight, couldn't he? He'd 
go out again and start without money start on a less pretentious business and 
make a go of it. He could not bear to let his mother continue having that 
look on her face, or let it come back again. 

But parents may be somewhat less ubiquitous. Very commonly the 
parent may allow the child considerable freedom in social relationships but 



THE REGION OF THE FAMILY 121 

take a strong stand about his choice of career or wife. In the following story, 
which is typical of this protocol, these two influences appear in the same story. 

The young man, Peter, had become a great doctor in his home town, and 
his parents had made great sacrifices to give him the best education possible. 
Peter had decided to take a vacation and go to the city to brush up on city 
medical ideas and news. While there he met a girl and got engaged. Sh* 
was the selfish debutante type and insisted that Peter give up his small town 
practice and move to the city where her father could set him up very well in 
his brokerage firm. Peter said he would go home, attend to some business 
and then come back to the city and be married. When he comes to tell 
his mother of his plans, she is very upset and will not take part in any o his 
wedding arrangements. She finally gives in and goes to the city for the 
wedding, but comes home afterward, broken-hearted because she believes Peter 
has given up his whole life. She refused to have anything to do with him, 
until two years later when there is a divorce and Peter comes home to resume 
his practice, a wiser man. 

Less commonly the parents, frustrated in their own aspirations, center 
their influence on one particular region in an effort to achieve through the 
child what they themselves have failed to achieve. They may want the child 
to marry a wealthy person, achieve fame, know the "right people," or, as in 
the story below, be accepted in a good college in the East. 

There was a certain young man who lived in a large family. His father was 
very overpowering and demanded that he do things just when his father 
wanted him to. Not much free choice. His father wanted more than ever foi 
him to be accepted in a good college in the East. His mother wanted him tc 
be accepted too. This is the one thing they were together on. Subjected all 
other petty grievances to gain that goal, and when this picture takes place, 
the boy just received an acceptance to M.I.T,, and is dreaming of all the 
tremendous future and the chance that he has been given and is positive that 
he will be successful. Everything turns out very beautifully, very successful. 

2. DURATION OF PARENTAL IMPACT 

Parental impact may be further considered in its temporal dimension. 
The parents may, for example, control the child's life completely in childhood 
and relinquish or lose this control in late adolescence or after the child has 
left home. But the child may never leave home and the control continues, 
or in the extreme instance the child may leave home and continue to suffer 
the intervention of the parents in his own home or turn again to the parent 
for guidance and support. The stories that follow illustrate variations in the 
duration of parental impact. 

The parent may be unwilling to relinquish control but the child on reach- 
ing adulthood may declare his independence. 



122 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

This girl is tired o having her family tell her what to do, just because she's 
living at home and they support her. She wants to be her own boss so she 
goes out and gets a job and comes home and tells them and moves her things 
out. Now she is making her own money and can be her own boss with no 
one to tell her what to do. 

On the other hand, the parent, although not completely willing, may 
relinquish control when the child reaches maturity. 

The young man's mother had planned on her son becoming a great physician 
as his father had been. After going through pre-medical school he realizes 
that he will never make a good surgeon for he cannot bear to see others in 
pain. He knows that he will be a great baseball player if he wants to devote 
his energy to it. He tells his mother of his great chance to become a member 
of a professional team. When he breaks the news to her, her dreams are 
shattered. But realizing that her son is at an age where he must make his 
own decisions, she consents. It of course hurts die son to grieve his mother 
but he must go ahead with the work he loves to become a great hero on 
the baseball diamond. Later the mother is to find joy in seeing her son 
being cheered by millions. 

But independence may be short lived, due not to parental intervention 
but to the individual's need for continued parental advice. 

As always, age and experience tried to guide youth in its lack of experience, 
and youth, not using the advice, found out the same truth the hard way. In 
this case the father had had the business and then retired, handing it over 
to his son. Instead of taking his father's tried and true advice, the son had 
plunged ahead with his new ideas without thinking first, and so gotten 
the business in a terrible mess. He was just now realizing his mistake, 
ashamed and furious at himself and the whole business and coming to his 
father for advice on how to straighten the business out. 

Or the relationship with the parent may be resumed at a later period, 
particularly when there are problems in the love region. The following is 
a story told by a man of 50. 

A worried man with a problem, trouble with his wife, is asking advice from 
his mother who is also perplexed and worried. He has come to her with his 
problem and she evidently does not know how to answer him. He is nervous 
and troubled. She cannot solve his problem. 

Finally, parental impact may continue throughout the life of the indi- 
vidual as in the following story. 

This man and his wife are having mother-in-law trouble. The man has 
always been under his mother's thumb, and now that he got married it isn't 
any different The wife can't stand the mother butting in all the time, but 
there isn't much she can do about it. When they have their first child the 
grandmother is there johnny on the spot to tell them how to raise the child. 



THE REGION OF THE FAMILY 123 

And then the thing that the wife was afraid of most happened. Her husband's 
father died, and the mother came to live with them. The husband wasn't too 
crazy for this, but what could he do it was his own mother, and she had 
no place else to go. They lived together for the rest of their lives, the 
husband henpecked by the mother and the wife, and they fighting each other. 
Amen. 

K. GENERALIZATION OF THE PARENT-CHILD RELATIONSHIP 
Perhaps one of the most important criteria in the determination of the 
importance of the family region is the degree of generalization of the parent- 
child relationship; the extent to which adults are cast in the role of parents; 
and the extent to which the individual's reactions to these surrogates are 
similar to his former reactions to his own parents. 

If the psychoanalytic assumption of the importance of the family in 
shaping adult attitudes is true, we should expect to find the individual's 
childhood reactions continuing unmodified toward parental surrogates and 
sibling surrogates in the social, work, and love relationships of later life. To 
the extent that important differences occur we cannot invoke the doctrine 
of simple repetition. It is our experience that such generalization of parental 
impact is indeed found, but that the universality of this process needs con- 
siderable qualification. 

Commonly, the narrator does not directly tell us the extent to which 
adults are cast in the role of parents or the extent to which his reactions to 
these parental surrogates are similar to his former reactions to his own parents. 
To determine this it is necessary to compare the stories in which the hero is 
a child with those in which he is an adult,* 

DIFFERENTIATION IN PORTRAYAL OF OLDER PEOPLE 

We must consider first the differentiation between the portrayal of parents 
and other adults in the entire protocol. Do older people have the same 
characteristics as the parents portrayed in the stories? We find examples 
ranging from complete similarity to complete dissimilarity. There are protocols 
in which some older adults are similar to parent figures described in the same 
protocol, but other adults bear no resemblance whatever to the parents. We 
have found cases where a parental surrogate has actually had an important 
influence on the young adult's life and in these cases nonparental figures in 
the TAT sometimes resemble the parent and sometimes resemble the parental 
surrogate who was actually important in their early adulthood. 

* The term "parent" will be used in this discussion to denote parents in stories in which 
the hero is a child. The terms "surrogate," "older adult," and "other adult" will be used 
interchangeably to denote adults in stories in which the hero is an adult. The use of the 
term surrogate in this way somewhat prejudges the issue, since individuals may react to 
older figures without regarding them as parent surrogates, but we are interested here in those 
figures who may in some cases be regarded as parent surrogates. 



124 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

THE HERO'S REACTION 

The continuities and discontinuities of the hero's reaction to parents and 
parent surrogates must also be examined. This is an independent variable, 
inasmuch as we find examples in which the parents and other adults may be 
portrayed as similar, but the hero reacts in one way to the parent and in 
another way to other adults. There are also cases where differences in the 
hero's reaction can occur only in response to those adults who treat him 
differently than he was treated by his parents. 

PERMEATION OF OTHER REGIONS 

Finally, we must examine the extent to which this generalization of 
parental impact has permeated other regions. In the extreme instance we find 
that parental impact has permeated all regions. The work region is a replica 
of the family region, the boss playing the role of the father or mother and 
the co-workers cast in the role of sibs. In the love region the family romance 
is again recapitulated; the characteristics of the wife are those of the mother 
and the response of the husband is that of the son. In the social region older 
figures and younger people are also portrayed as if the human family were a 
replica of the individual's own family. This degree of generalization, however, 
is very rarely found in TAT stories. More commonly we find that most 
regions are relatively free of such generalization, but in one particular region 
work s love or social relationships the family constellation is recapitulated. Or 
frequently this type of generalization may occur only under special conditions; 
the usual adult reaction in the love relationship may differ from the child's 
reaction, but wherever a rival is introduced, the hero may regress to the reaction 
described in the stories of his childhood. Another example is seen in a protocol 
in which two different stories described the hero at work. In one the employer 
bore no resemblance to the father described in other stories and the individual 
worked well; but in the other the employer was described in much the same 
terms as the father and the hero lost his job for inefficiency. Here we are 
told in effect that when the environment resembles that of the individual's 
unpleasant childhood, he responds in the old ways, but if the environment is 
different, he responds differently. Thus we find individuals capable, in varying 
degrees, of distinguishing the realities of the present environment from the 
characteristics of their childhood environment. While achievement of this dif- 
ferentiation is the explicit goal of psychoanalytic therapy it is not an achieve- 
ment peculiar to therapeutic intervention; differentiation of this type is one 
of the salient characteristics of the learning process in normal development. 

The following examples illustrate varying degrees of generalization of 
the parent-child relationship. In the first story, told by an adolescent girl, 



THE REGION OF THE FAMILY T25 

we see within a single story the generalization of the family romance to her 
own later love life. 

This young girl is a wayward youngster about 20, parents divorced, and she 
has always been left on her own. She was brought up in the poor section 
and had to shift for herself when she was too young. Extremely devoted to 
her father, intensified her hatred of her mother. At the present time she 
had lost her father and has now is in grief and tears. She meets this 
young man and falls for him. She's a very jealous person and her love 
is so great she can't bear to see him go around with anyone else. The 
usual love triangle. He finally tells her off and seeing that she has a 
weak character, she commits suicide. 

In the following story told by another individual we see again, within 
a single story, the generalization from a broken home to general social rela- 
tionships. Generalization in this story, however, is not to adulthood but to 
another region in childhood. 

The boy was sent to preparatory school not with, the intentions of furthering 
his education but mainly because his parents thought it would be better for 
him. His family life had been a sad one. His mother died when he was 
seven years of age. . . . Later his father married again. After three 
years elapsed they became separated and finally divorced. While at school the 
boy imagined all sorts of things; that no one liked him or cared what he did. 
So he ran away. When the thick of night started to fall, he sought shelter in 
an old deserted barn. Here he spent the night. When signs of morning came, 
he set out for some unknown destination. 

Generalization of the parent-child relationship may be tempered by later 
development and maturation, as in the following two stories, which illustrate 
the modification of the original mother-son relationship in adult marriage. 

This boy had been playing out on the street with the other boys. His 
mother had called him a number of times but he hadn't heeded her. Finally 
she went after him and brought him home. She administered a sound 
spanking because he hadn't obeyed her. Here he is probably crying with 
tears of remorse. Tears of remorse maybe for a little while, then thinking 
how unfair for a boy not to be able to play all he wanted without having 
mothers spank such a big boy as he. In a little while his mother will 
probably call him to dinner, or perhaps lunch. Now he'll probably be a little 
more obedient, perhaps wipe his tears with his sleeve. His mother will tell 
him to go to the bathroom and wash up. He'll go without hesitation and 
come back to the dinner table a little bit refreshed, because of the cold water 
he probably washed his face with. He would probably eat slowly at first, 
gather up a little speed as the experience begins to wear off. His mother 
will probably admonish him not to do such things again, and he'll probably 
finish off a tall glass of milk with alacrity. By then his resentment will have 
gone. 



126 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

This could mean so many things I don't know where to start. There's been 
a flood or it could be a fire. A fire would probably be more logical. 
Probably across the street from where these people are living. It's burning 
pretty badly by now. There may be somebody up in the building. The 
husband is very anxious to go up and help. His wife naturally wants to 
hold him back because there is a great element of danger involved. From 
the look of determination in his eyes and mouth, he'll probably put her 
gently aside in a little while and rush over and see what he can do. The 
fireman will stop him anyway, although he'll try desperately to cross the 
line. In a little while the fireman will have the fire under control and 
he'll walk back to his wife. No he won't walk back to his wife because 
she'll have been right there, have run over after him. The thing will turn 
out to be not as bad as they had feared. There'll be no loss of life, but the 
entire property will be ruined. He will offer his help to rebuild the home 
of the family. 

Important continuities underlie the son's relation to his mother and his 
present relations with his wife. Both wife and mother are ubiquitous and 
wish to control the hero's behavior but there are important differences. The 
mother's dominance is at once more active, "Finally she went after him and 
brought him home," and more coercive, "She administered a sound spanking 
because he hadn't obeyed her." The wife however, "wants to hold him back" 
not because she insists on obedience to her dictates but, "because there is a 
great element of danger involved." She is less insistent and more considerate 
of the hero's welfare. She too will "run after him" but she will not drag him 
back and spank him for disobedience. The son ignores his mother when she 
calls him but after a spanking, "he'll probably be a little more obedient." He 
is overtly obedient to his mother's command that he wash before dinner, "He'll 
go without hesitation." The seeds of rebellion appear on the covert level in 
childhood, "tears of remorse maybe for a little while, then thinking how unfair 
for a boy not to be able to play all he wanted without having mothers spank 
such a big boy a$ he." By the time he finishes dinner his resentment will have 
gone. Since his resentment of this maternal dominance is freely admitted 
into consciousness, we might well have expected to find the same hero as a 
husband limiting the exercise of dominance by his wife and this is the case, 
"he'll probably put her gently aside in a little while and rush over and see 
what he can do." The continuity of behavior attributed to mother and wife 
appears in his afterthought: "and he'll walk back to his wife. No he won't 
walk back to his wife because she'll have been right there, have run over 
after him." 

These stories are representations of actual differences between his original 
relationship with his mother and his contemporary marital relationship. The 
influence of the original relationship looms large but the seeds of its change 



THE REGION OF THE FAMILY 127 

were apparent in those feelings which the hero could not express towards his 
mother but was ultimately capable of expressing towards his wife. 

The continuity between attitude toward the parent and later parental 
surrogate in the example above was marked.. Less continuity exists in the 
following case. There is some generalization of a negativistic attitude toward 
work imposed by the parents, but this reaction is considerably modified in 
adulthood. Let us examine this relationship as it appears in the following 
stories. 

Little Jack was given his violin and told to practice for two hours, his usual 
practice time. Jack was wedded to his violin and would have rather played 
it than anything else, but, sometimes well he wishes that he didn't have to 
give up the whole afternoon to practicing, practicing, practicing. Neverthe- 
less, he began his scales and diligently tried to make himself a great violinist. 
For some reason, everything seemed to go wrong today, he couldn't play 
anything right. The voices of children playing on the grass suddenly 
captivated him. First he watched them, then he would look at his violin, 
wishing that he might play perfectly. Jack was finally tempted to join the 
other children for a short while. They played hard together for the rest 
of the afternoon, but suddenly he returned to his violin and found that for 
some reason, he could almost play perfectly. He had eaten his cake and 
had it too. 

Ellen was on her way home from school where she had been diligently 
teaching all day. How good it seemed to get out in the open and walk 
home after her long day. "Why do I keep this up?" she thought. ''Why 
don't I work on a farm as my friends and the rest of my family do? They 
don't have to correct papers at night! They don't have lessons to give to 
twenty-five rude, indifferent brats. Wouldn't it be fun to plough and be 
with the animals all day?" "No" she finally thought after some meditation, 
'I'm just not the type to be a farmerette the children here need to be 
educated, and if I can do it, and do a little good for someone else, I think I 
had better stick to it." 

In childhood the hero is "wedded to his violin." Despite this he wishes 
he "didn't have to give up the whole afternoon to practicing, practicing, prac- 
ticing." He conforms to a certain point but finally is tempted to join the other 
children for a short while. On his return he "found for some reason he could 
play almost perfectly. He had eaten his cake and had it too." As a young 
adult that part of a task which is routine and mechanical is still rejected, 
"They don't have to correct papers at night!" But there is an important 
change in the consequences of this dissatisfaction. In the first story the child 
yields to temptation and on his return achieves his ends magically. In the 
second story the wish to do something else is never translated into action 
primarily because the heroine realizes the need of the children "to be educated 



128 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

and if I can do it, and do a litde good for someone else, I think I had better 
stick to it." Not only is the wish not translated into action but the reason 
for this has changed. The older heroine is possessed of an ideal of service to 
others which was altogether lacking in childhood when work was done either 
because the parents insisted or because the child "would rather have played 
it [violin] than anything else." Further there is much greater realism in 
the second story. The heroine realizes that no longer can one have "eaten 
his cake and had it too." The attitudes of childhood have generalized into 
adult life only to the extent that there is dissatisfaction with boring means-end 
activity. No longer, however, is she a child resisting parental dominance but 
an adult governed by adult ideals. 

In the preceding examples we have seen various degrees of generalization 
of the parent-child relationship both to another region in childhood and to 
adult attitudes. The examples which follow illustrate variations in the type 
of generalization. 

Childhood may be important to the individual apart from the parent-child 
relationship and childhood attitudes not involving the child's relation to the 
parents may be generalized to adult life. Children of high intelligence, for 
example, may be sufficiently rewarded by the free play of their own curiosity 
to turn them in the direction of anyone who is intellectually stimulating in 
childhood and later life alike. 

The following are stories told by a young woman whose major energies 
since childhood have been directed towards an eager exploration of the physical 
world and science in general. The parents could do no more than expose 
her to books and people who could answer her questions. As a child these 
weie the people who mattered, and as a young adult this is still the case: 

Once when Frederick was a very, very, very little boy his father took him along 
to hear a famous violinist play at the Opera House. During the performance 
and on the way home, Frederick had said nothing nor did he for days 
afterwards. Finally one afternoon his father, in his study, looked up to 
find the huge door being slowly pushed open by litde Frederick. The boy 
came over to him and, with the same look of yearning in his eyes, asked 
if he could please, please have a violin of his own; that to play a violin was 
all he wanted ever in the wide world. His father didn't say much, but 
soon a big box came for Frederick. Hardly daring to hope, he opened it 
slowly, and lo there it was his own violin. He took it out gingerly and 
lovingly and set it on the table before him and just looked at it with that 
same loving, longing look. And do you know what little Frederick is 
now? After studying and studying and studying, he became one of our 
most famous violinists. 

He was a funny litde old professor, as absent-minded as they come and his 
course as stiff as the very devil to pass, but he was the favorite of the 
whole college. You could count on him for a warm and cheery greeting as 



THE REGION OF THE FAMILY 129 

he passed, even if he didn't know you. In fact they all thought he forgot 
who he did know. He had been at the college for years and years sort of 
grown up with it. As long as anyone had known him there his hair had 
been tousled as if he had just gotten up and forgotten to brush it. It was 
a funny thing, you pay him a visit for the purpose of hearing him ramble on 
and on about any subject under the sun, in the hope of absorbing some of 
his knowledge, but in the end, you always found yourself telling him of your- 
selt, your home, your girl, your ideals and ambitions, while he listened 
attentively. Yes, he was beloved by the whole college, and when he died 
it was as if a great part one of the most important parts of the college, 
had just ceased to be. It left an irreparable gap in the lives of the students 
as well as the professors. 

Joe had always liked to read story books. He could as a rule be found 
curled up in the large old leather chair in the library completely enraptured 
by some story. Whether it was & good thing or not was a question because 
the stories affected him deeply. For days after reading a good book, he 
would be in a dreary state, imagining he was a pirate of yore, or an 
enchanting prince, etc. But Joe was a sensitive boy, affected by a great 
many things. If an odd stranger came to the house, or if Joe went to the 
museum, or just took a walk in the park among the birds and flowers, he 
could be seen looking up with wide dark eyes in wonderment Many people 
didn't understand him at all. At the present time, Joe was thinking of the 
picture of that barbarian operation that was painted in his mind by the pirate 
story. How could they have really done such an awful thing as cut a man's 
stomach without giving him any anesthetic. But they did it in a kind way, 
he knew that, they just didn't know anything else to do. But now that 
people did know how to help humanity and save lives without pain, he 
wanted to do it! That was his ambition the thing he must do. Think 
how those poor devils had suffered then, and to even think how they would 
suffer about the same way today if enough people didn't become doctors 
and make sure that humanity was all saved from unnecessary pain and 
loss of life. As soon as he gets older, he would go to medical school. 

The individual in his generalization of the parent-child relationship may 
reverse the roles. He does not act toward parental surrogates as he acted 
toward his parents, but he becomes the same type of parent and treats his 
children as his parents treated him. 

Uh. . . . This girl lives in the midwest and uh, her folks own a farm. They 
belong to the middle class and they have a hard time keeping it up. Both 
her mother and father have to work hard, but they do their best to give 
her an education. She appreciates this and works hard studying and working 
around home so she won't be too much of a liability. She isn't dressed as 
fashionably and her hair isn't arranged in a modern style, but she wants 
to work hard in spite of the small inconveniences so that she'll be able to 
repay them for all they've done for her. Uh. . . . She has a strong attachment 
for her family and after she finishes her education and works for a while, 
she marries a farmer and raises a family and gives them the benefits her 
family has given her. 



I^O THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

Occasionally we see a case in which two parent-child relationships have 
been generalized because either a foster parent or a parental surrogate has inter- 
vened in the life of the child. The impress of both the original parent and 
the foster parent may be traced in the TAT stories of such an individual; 
the later influence is superimposed on the earlier, with a blending of generaliza- 
tion. The following stones are those of a young woman whose mother 
divorced and remarried. In the first story we are told of her reactions to 
her new father. 

Poor little John he had been playing so hard all day and now he was 
tired. His life was full of fun all day long, but something was lacking. He 
didn't know what it was. It wasn't a lack of love from his mother, but she 
couldn't be with him much. Now that his Dad was dead, she had to work. 
Yes that was it he missed his father. He missed their hunting trips, the 
games they used to play, everything that his father had meant to him. He 
wanted his dad. Then who was this man that had been coming to see 
his mother. He resented his coming and showing so much attention to his 
mother. And she didn't pay so much attention to John when he came over. 
The great blow came when John's mother told him that she was going to 
marry that man. Now, he thought, I will be all alone. But things weren't 
like that. His new dad was a swell guy. They took camping trips and 
even his mom went along. Yes, he would be all right. Maybe he's not 
such a bad guy after all. And things did turn out very well and he even 
got more attention from this new comer than he did from his father. He 
found out in later years that his own father hadn't died after all but had 
been sent to prison and so things did turn out for the best. 

This is a literal representation of her attitude toward her original father, 
the reception of her foster father and the restructuration of her attitude toward 
both fathers after she had achieved a secure relationship with the foster parent. 

A less direct expression of this change is reflected in the story to card i. 

Poor Tommy! He had been taking violin lessons for four years now and 
although he was quite the child prodigy, he just learned that he couldn't go 
on with his lessons. Tommy, as young as he was, understood this and so in 
front of people he gave the appearance that he was tired of playing his 
violin, but when he was alone he would lay the violin on the table and look 
at it with longing eyes. When everyone was away from the house, he 
would go into his room and pick it up and play until he heard someone come 
back but they were the same old pieces over and over again. However, one 
day he was playing his violin and a great musician heard him and was 
simply shocked by the beauty of what he heard. Everything turned out all 
right then because the musician made Tommy his protege* and Tommy got 
his fill of the thing he loved to do. 

The effect of this change in her life and its generalization to later attitudes 
is striking. In almost every story told by this young woman the. hero is faced 
with apparently insuperable difficulties which disappear as if by magic. The 



THE REGION OF THE FAMILY 13! 

scar of her early childhood, indicated by the heroine o the next, story who 
had been a cripple since she was born, has not been forgotten. She does 
indeed generalize on the basis of her early experience to the extent that she 
expects to meet many situations as unpleasant as her early home environment. 
But the experience of magical rescue has also been generalized. The resultant 
is a confluence of both forces in her contemporary expectations. These joint 
expectations are reflected in the following stories. 

Marie has been a cripple since she was born and although she didn't show 
it outwardly, inside she cringed when she saw other children, running and 
playing. But then a miracle happened something was done that most doctors 
said couldn't be done. She was cured. Naturally she was overjoyed but 
her joy came to light when her father came to the hospital and found that 
she was going to be well again. They sat enfolded in each other's arms, 
sobbing with joy and both talking at once. And then there was silence. 
Upon being asked what was wrong, she said that she didn't know how to 
play because she had always been alone. The other children had always 
made fun of her. But this was remedied by their moving to another city 
and here Marie became one of the leaders and she didn't stop here. She 
went on and on and when she grew up she was one of the most successful 
people of her day. 

Chris and Cherry had only been married a year. Yes, they were poor and 
things weren't turning out the way they expected. No, two couldn't live as 
cheaply as one. But Chris was proud and wouldn't let his wife work. She 
was silent about the affair knowing his feelings on the subject, but she just 
couldn't stand this poverty any longer. She went to him and told him that 
she wanted a divorce she could get along by herself. Both their hearts were 
breaking because they loved each other very much, but pride came into the 
scene too. So Cherry left Chris. She didn't get the divorce, but she lived 
in another town and got a job and lived a little better. She had been 
living alone now for about six months, damning the day she left Chris and 
wanting him so badly. But the very next day, Chris came -to her apartment 
and after the polite conversation was finished he blurted out that he had a 
very good position now and he wanted her to come back to him. All 
ended very happily and they went back together. He was successful in his 
business and she had her own little home and all that she never thought 
were possible. 

Mrs. Kelly answered the door and there she found a young man who looked 
rather ill at ease. After inquiring if she was Mrs. Kelly, mother of Jane, he 
told her he had some rather unfortunate news. They had been in an 
accident and Jane had been hurt "Oh, my baby" was all that Mrs. Kelly 
could say. She couldn't leave the house because her husband was upstairs in 
bed and he needed her near him every minute. What could she do? She 
knew that Jane needed her too. The young man told her that everything 
had been taken care of and Jane was resting quietly in the hospital. Every- 
thing did turn out all right and soon Jane was back home again feeling just 
as spry as she was before. 



132 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

L. CHANGES IN THE PARENT-CHILD RELATIONSHIP 

The parent-child relationship is not static; it undergoes change as both 
the parent and the child grow older. This change may or may not involve 
a change in the relative importance of the family for the individual. We 
may, for example, find in one protocol stories in which parental dominance 
is increasingly rejected as the child grows older and the parents completely 
lose their influence over him, while in another protocol the parents may be 
dominant but as the child grows older temper their dominance, offering 
advice only when the child seeks it and in this way continue to exert influence 
on him. 

Any changes in the influence of the family during the life of the indi- 
vidual may be directly mentioned within a single story. This is common 
in the stories of those whose relationship with the family has been marked 
by dramatic changes which have not escaped the attention of the individual. 
He is aware of the changes and this awareness is reflected in his stories. 

Frequently, however, these changes are so gradual and each one so slight 
that the total change is unnoticed. Wherever these changes have been of such 
a nature as to escape the awareness of the person they are rarely reflected in 
any single story. We can discover whether or not such changes are present 
in the stories only by a comparison of all the stories told in which a parent 
and child are the principal characters. In this comparison two important 
questions should be kept in mind. The first is the obvious question, Is there 
a difference in the parent-child relationship when the hero is of different ages? 
The second is of equal importance, Can we be certain that the difference we 
find is a junction of the age of the hero in the stories'? 

In other words, is the difference in the age of the hero the only factor 
to which the change in the parent-child relationship may be traced or is there 
some other factor, perhaps unrecognized, to which this change may be 
attributed? If, for example, we were to find in a protocol one story in which 
a young child is submissive to parental dominance and another story in which 
a young adult rebels against parental dominance, we might assume that as the 
child grew older he became more rebellious. If, however, we were to find 
a third story in which a young child is rebellious to parental dominance we 
would question whether our assumption that this rebellion was a function 
of the age of the hero was true. We would then have to re-examine the 
stories for some factor other than the age of the hero to account for this 
difference in the hero's reaction. 

This type of analysis was employed by the writer only after several years 
of what he must now regard as serious misinterpretation of TAT material. 
When confronted, in the past, with a protocol in which the hero expressed 
intense love for his mother in one story and equally intense hatred for her 



THE REGION OF THE FAMILY 133 

in another or rebelled against parental dominance in one story and was sub- 
missive in another, the writer assumed this to be evidence of an ambivalent 
attitude toward the parent or toward parental dominance. Or, to take another 
example, if the boy in picture i regarded the violin as the instrument through 
which he would achieve fame and worked hard toward this end, whereas 
the hero of another story was lazy, preferring the life of a hobo, to any kind 
of work, this was regarded as evidence of. ambivalence toward "work" in 
general. This may be the case but such fine gradations of delineation of the 
parent-child relationship were found in so many protocols that the writer 
grew suspicious and reanalyzed a long series of protocols previously inter- 
preted in this way. It then appeared that the majority of differences in the 
parent-child relationship found within each protocol could be better explained 
as an outgrowth of the individual's development. That, if the age of the hero 
in each story was considered, there was to be found in the microcosm of 
phantasy the impress of the individual's actual developmental sequence. If 
this proves to be the case the TAT offers us, in a two hour period, the possi- 
bility of tracing patterns of growth which would otherwise require years of 
laborious observation and study. This type of exploration might be more 
easily accomplished if a special series of pictures were employed depicting 
the parent and child together in a temporal series graded from infancy to 
maturity. 

If one assumes that the TAT stories reflect the individual's personality, 
it is a small step to explain the further reflection of the developmental sequence. 
When presented with a picture of a boy facing a violin, the story is made 
congruous with a small boy's life and therefore with the individual's life as 
a child, and the same is true with older characters in succeeding pictures. If 
the individual tells us anything about himself, there is no reason to doubt that 
he can project himself into the past as easily as he can divulge his present 
preoccupations. 

Determination of the generality of this type of projection presents an 
empirical problem of no small dimension, but we believe that further inquiry 
along these lines will be rewarding. 

MODIFICATIONS 

In the experience- of the writer it is certain that such projection into th * 
past is not in every case a "true" reflection of the sequence of development. 
There are at least two modifications of this mechanism limiting the inferences 
which may be made from this tvpe of analysis. 

First, there are instances in which important factors shaping the develop- 
ment of the adolescent may be projected back into childhood as though these 
same factors had been in operation earlier than their actual appearance in the 



1 34 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

individual's development. This type of modification will be discussed in detail 
in the case of Helmler. 

Second, the individual's projection into the past may be a function of his 
present condition. This is a special case of a general phenomenon; it is well 
known that the picture of both the past and the future is to some extent 
a function of the individual's present condition. Thus frequently when a 
person is depressed he finds it impossible to believe that he ever was happy 
or that he ever will be happy again. We shall see in the case of Eggman how 
the present may color the past in the TAT, and when an individual's immediate 
problems seem insoluble or threaten to overwhelm him he may see all char- 
acters, whatever their age, struggling with the same problems. 

This second type of modification places even more serious limitation on 
the inferences which may be made about the developmental sequence, but this 
is somewhat counterbalanced by the fact that it is easier to detect if we are 
familiar with the individual's present state, whereas only through a knowledge 
of the past history of the individual can we be certain whether or not the TAT 
faithfully mirrors the sequence of development or whether the image is 
blurred by the modification first mentioned. 

THE CASE OF HELMLER 

Past History 

In a study by White, Tomkins, and Alper (106) of the personality of an 
individual whom we called Helmler, there is an example of telescoping in 
TAT stories. Helmler was frankly unimpressed with his father but very 
devoted to his mother until the age of 6. By the age of 6 this cathexis had 
been tempered by the presence of his brother five and one half years older 
who was "a long way off a man" and who commanded the respect of both 
parents. Helmler was "irritated" because his older brother's "judgement was 
listened to, and mine was not." He was always "the baby" and dominated 
and teased by his older brother. Helmler chose him as his earliest masculine 
model: "When I was quite young I followed closely in my brother's footsteps." 
In adolescence, however, he was somewhat disillusioned his brother turned 
out to be a "grind." His relationship with his mother was further complicated 
by the problem of the control of bodily impulses. A slip at the age of 3 or 4 
in an automobile was the occasion of humiliation sufficient to create a dread 
of automobiles, overcome only at the age of eighteen when he first learned 
to drive. It also created a strong sympathy for "dogs not yet housebroken." 
Continence, he reports, was a matter of pride, but there were several slips 
(enuretic) at 8 years, which occasioned ridicule from his brother. He also 
reports being "fortunately scared out of" thumb-sucking at 4 or 5. Fingernail 
biting continued unt'l he was n; his mother would slap his hands for this. 



THE REGION OF THE FAMILY 135 

Control of bodily impulses was not easy and its significance was somewhat 
emphasized by the presence of the older brother who was so superior to the 
"baby" in this as well as other accomplishments. At this time, too, a rich phan- 
tasy life appears, charged with anxiety. He had phantasies, of being observed 
by God and great fear of punishment when he did anything wrong. His 
mother moreover was beginning to substitute dominance and more difficult 
expectations in the place of unquestioned admiration. The baby role, while 
denuded of its rewards, was nonetheless ascribed to him by/ mother, father, 
and brother. Helmler was more than willing to drop the role, but his mother 
was not so willing to relax her nurturant maternal dominance, and so Helmler 
seems to have experienced, at the age of 6, a striving for independence more 
typically adolescent. He sought the solution to this problem in the cultivation 
of his age equals and began to stay away from home. "I played mostly with 
people at school, not around home. That's my mother's chief complaint, that 
I was never at home. If we weren't playing we were involved in something 
down at school. I never hang around home. I never could understand being 
homesick. I was always glad to get out in the country in summer." 

Although he acknowledges his mother's influence, "copied her likes and 
dislikes tastes, almost unconsciously clothes, as petty as that music, movies, 
small things like that I've noticed . . . never stopped to figure out larger things, 
probably more influence than I know," his struggle for individuality issues 
in the following statement in "his autobiography: "But by and large, I just 
grew without anything that seemed to me to be the conscious influences on 
the part of my parents." 

Separation from the family was for some time a successful resolution of 
his problem. He was an active member of the neighborhood gang and at 
school "managed to keep honor grades throughout and stood close to the top. 
They elected me class president a few times and I liked to act in the little 
skits and plays which we presented." But though he sought out his age equals, 
this relationship could easily be spoiled if it derived in any way from his 
parents. "The only thing I can remember is resenting friends when my 
parents tried to make them for me: here's a nice little boy why don't you 
play with him? I didn't like that." 

On entering high school, "I began to take an interest in school affairs. 
In school politics I attained some success, became president of the assembly, 
of the honor society, of various social science clubs, editor of the year book, 
and generally active in student affairs." The high point of this period of his 
life came with a scholarship to a summer camp dedicated to the development 
of democratic leadership. "The boys ran the camp as far as they could, with 
expert advice from a fine staff. There was equipment for the development 
of all sorts of talent, and encouragement to all. Three summers at this camp 



136 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

had a tremendous effect on my personality. I made about fifteen close friends 
whom I still call close friends. And in an environment that lacked the watch- 
fulness of home I began to \now what it -felt liJ(e to shift for oneself. \Ve were 
all given responsibility and in my last years I was given a good deal of 
authority as one of the camp leaders." Helmler's vision of the future, as 
described in his autobiography, further underlines the importance of this 
experience in shaping his life. "I visualize a world of a few large comple- 
mentary powers who will be responsible to some competent world organiza- 
tion. I want the United States to play the chief role in this world, and 
therefore I want the United States to be ruled by competent men. I hope 
for a world governed by the modern version of the philosopher-king the well 
educated statesman-politician who is ultimately responsible to the people. I 
look for a socialistic state where wealth is equalized, opportunities spread, and 
yet where man is not reduced to the average. The 'wise and the just' must be 
given enough power to rule effectively but not despotically. I like to picture 
myself in some position of importance in government, such as justice in a 
high court. I would like to have some power in enforcing and interpreting 
this complicated social system which would be necessary." 

Helrnler's adjustment during these years sprang from native intellectual 
and social ability, but his striving was greatly reinforced by both familial and 
endopsychic rejection of the "baby role," made especially intolerable by sibling 
rivalry. Equally important, however, were the substantial rewards foi his 
independent strivings outside the family. 

TAT Stories and Interpretation 
In the light of this past history, let us examine a few of his TAT stories: 

Thi's picture would mean, ah, brings up the idea of some sort of a story 
for children perhaps on the tale of a little boy who had some sort of musical 
talent which he, he showed at an awfully early age, child prodigy perhaps in, 
in the violin, and ah a through some chance I will have to work that out 
some awfully, ah fortunate happening, for instance accidentally, accidental 
fiddling with a great violin ah, brought him to the attention of some first- 
rate musician, took an interest in him and, ah here we have him gazing at 
this violin, dreaming of how happy he would be if he could have the 
advantages of good training and possess this violin, and this violin becomes 
the, the, oh, supreme good thing that ever happened to him and, ah we 
might have the rest of the story built around a. struggle for some sort of 
recognition against, with a conflict of a familv perhaps and, ah lack of money 
and, ah, the fact that he is such an awfully young fellow and nobody will 
pay any attention to him and that, at the end perhaps we could have him 
being awarded as the result of some recital a first-rate violin and ah, perhaps 
opening up a path toward development of his talent but not, nothing very 



THE REGION OF THE FAMILY 137 

complete or conclusive in the line of success. I don't mean that the end 
should have him a great violinist acclaimed by thousands or anything like that. 

I walked into this apparent hole in the ground and looking with a good 
deal of wonder when a man walked up to me and said, "Take this and any 
time you need any help why merely ask it and take your tool and use it 
whenever danger approaches." I just walked along this .... Well, it was 
some sort of a shiny metal instrument and moon-shaped with various emboss- 
ings on it almost like a shield a small one. And I walked along through, 
through what seemed to be the path of life passing these various dangers 
and for each one assuming different exteriors according to my need and 
protection, sometimes shell of an armadillo, at other times the spines of a 
porcupine and the scales of a fish and all sorts of horrible obstacles, some- 
times webbed feet sometimes to cross water, other times feet equipped for 
climbing over huge stones. Finally the path went around, turned around and 
led to a, came to a fork in the road one side seemed to point outward 
toward earth and continuation of life and the other one toward more wander- 
ings and more knowledge, more experience in the mysteries that I had 
been through. I don't know which one I took, though. Do I have to 
decide that? [Laughs] . . . Well, I took the one out to the earth and thought 
perhaps I could make use of some of the allegory and knowledge that I had 
picked up in my trip. 

Oh Mr. Browdin was a share-cropper down in Alabama and he found that 
his situation was growing rapidly worse and worse and worse as years 
went on and he, and he found, somehow he never seemed to be able to 
make any sort of a profit, any sort of a subsistence level existence out of his 
land. His land was always bad and somehow or other there was always 
something coming up, drought or, or sickness or some special need each year 
or some new tax always took away any extra money that he might have. 
Finally there is a, finally there is quite a bit of excitement around their village 
when a radical labor agitator comes in and tries to organize the tenant 
farmers into some sort of union. There is a lawsuit and the planter tries to 
compel this, this union to go under and the union behind this ah, find Mr. 
Browdin is one of the best speaking witnesses of their group so they borrow 
a, borrow a clean collar and clean shirt and clean suit for him and bring him into 
court and we find him getting instructions from the union lawyer as to how he 
should act on the witness stand. He gets on the stand and pleads eloquently, ah, 
for some sort of rehabilitation for his group. As the result of a successful trial, 
ah the, ah, tenant farmers do form a, a substantial organization which gets 
diem some improvements and a few guarantees for the next year, and in the 
last scene we have Mr. Browdin and his wife in the men's store buying their 
own suit and shirt. 

Common to each of these stories is Helmler's concern with the achieve- 
ment of success. In the first story it is "a struggle for some sort of recognition 
against, with a conflict of a family perhaps." In the second story, "thought 
perhaps I could make use of some of the allegory and knowledge that I had 
picked up in my trip," and in the final story it is through the hero's efforts 



138 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION. TES1 

that there is a "successful trial" and the "tenant farmers do form a substantial 
organization which gets them some improvements and a few guarantees for 
the next year." Common also to these stories are the conditions favorable to 
this achievement. In the first story it is through the fortunate intervention 
of a benevolent and gifted outsider ("brought him to the attention of some 
first-rate musician, took an interest in him") and he is later "awarded as 
the result of some recital a first-rate violin, and ah, perhaps opening up a 
path toward development of his talent." In the second story an unknown 
man "walked up to me and said 'take this and any time you need any help 
why merely ask it and take your tool and use it whenever danger approaches.' " 
And in the third story, "a radical labor agitator comes in and tries to organize 
the tenant farms into some sort of union . . . and we find him getting instruc- 
tions from the union lawyer as to how he should act on the witness" stand." 

Success is achieved through competence but competence requires the 
instruction and help of one who is already competent. Further, the instructor 
is an outsider, certainly someone outside the family. We can understand 
this in light of Helmler's past history. Most of his successes have been 
achieved outside the family and with the help of others. His success among 
his age equals, his experience in the summer camp, and his scholarship to 
Harvard were achieved "in a struggle for some sort of recognition against, 
with a conflict of a family, perhaps." But the crucial discrepancy in this 
projection backward into the story of the boy and the violin is that he did not 
receive this external help in his struggle against the family at the time when 
he was first breaking away from the family, at the age of 6. As we have 
seen, it was only when he reached summer camp as an adolescent that "in an 
environment that lacked the watchfulness of home I began to know what it 
felt like to shift for oneself." But this gratuitous assistance from the wealthy 
benefactor who supported the camp is projected into his earlier struggles for 
recognition against his family. 

In Helmler's imagination this difference is of small import. He is telling 
us in effect, in all of these stories, that through the help of the competent 
outsider he has been enabled to maintain his individuality against the family. 
It is, however, important for the theory of interpretation to differentiate 
between phantasy and the actual course of events if we are to use the TAT 
as an instrument for diagnosis of the developmental sequence. 

In addition to the telescoping of adolescent experience seen in the case 
of Helmler, this particular theme may represent a wish-fulfillment phantasy. 
The theme of the fairy godmother, universal in folklore, appears to represent 
the fulfillment of a need common to all children. Markmann (55) in her 
study of the relationship between TAT stories and the past history of the 
storyteller found that stories told to picture i most frequently reflected the 



THE REGION OF THE FAMILY 139 

individual's actual past history. However, the benefactor theme in which 
the child, through the help of a benefactor, was enabled to succeed showed 
the lowest correlation with the actual past history. This was found in one 
out of every three stories to have no basis in the past history of the individual 
who told such a story. Whereas other themes told to the same picture faith- 
fully represented the childhood of the storyteller. Further research into the 
types of themes most commonly representative of the individual's actual child- 
hood is required if we are to employ the TAT in the study of developmental 
sequences. 

THE CASE OF EGGMAN 

In the following case of a man of 50 whose problems were very acute 
and seemingly insoluble, we find attribution of his symptoms to the boy in 
picture 3. 

A tired child is sleeping on the floor in a sitting position with one hand and 
his head resting on a bench. The child has evidently fallen asleep while 
playing on the floor. The child will awaken sooner or later and resume his 
playing. 

Inability to keep awake was not typical of this individual as a child, 
but is his present symptom. 

Analysis of the temporal dimension must then proceed with caution, since 
there may be a telescoping of the temporal series in backward projections, 
juxtaposing elements from later life with those of early childhood, or there 
may be a complete restructuration of the past in terms of an immediate situa- 
tion whose pressure is so massive that even the distant past is reinterpreted 
in terms of it. But despite these limitations we have found much .evidence 
to support the hypothesis that stories told to different pictures frequently do, 
in fact, represent a reliable picture of the individual's developmental sequence. 
Further exploration of these differences in the protective mechanism would 
in our opinion cast much light on general personality dynamics. 

THE CASE OF MARNA 

Let us turn to a few of these cases where external evidence supports 
evidence based on the TAT stories. Consider the following sequence: 

The small boy hates to play his violin and his mother has made him practice 
because she hopes that someday he'll be a great musician. He gets so angry 
that he breaks the violin and then is sorry because he knows he'll' get 
whipped and sure enough he does 'cause it was a genuine Stradivarius, 

The boy has come home to tell his mother about His marriage he was out 
on a drunken spree and married the town's bad girl. She is heartbroken and 
refuses to allow him to bring home the girl. He has always been dominated 



140 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

by his mother and is very unhappy at her anger and commits suicide. The 
mother realizing her failing brings home the wife and makes a lady out 
of her. 

The girl has been so watched by her parents that she feels herself going 
crazy. She has to tell them to the minute where she goes and what she 
does. Then her mind collapses and she spends years in an institution before 
she becomes normal again. 

A man is a great artist and is suddenly hit by a mad hysteria to kill he 
shows this in his picture that he paints showing one old man lunging at 
another then his desire leaves him and he knows that anything bad he 
feels can just be brought out in his paintings and he'll never do real wrong. 

In the first of these stories the hero is a "small boy," in the second a "boy," 
in the third a "girl," and in the final story, a "man." The differences corre- 
sponding to these differences in the age of the hero are representative of 
similar changes in the personality of the young woman who told these stories. 

They represent, roughly, childhood and early and late adolescence. The 
fourth story has been included, although there is no reference to the child- 
parent relationship, for the light it throws on the later sequelae of this 
relationship. 

i. Changes in Parental Dominance 

The first three stories show a parent-child relationship based on domi- 
nance. But there are changes in time. In the first story the child suffers 
a dominance somewhat limited in scope and intensity. The mother "has made 
him practice" and later whips him. In the second story "he has always been 
dominated by his mother" and the mother "refuses to allow him to bring 
home the girl," but in the third story dominance has increased even more, 
in both scope and intensity, "The girl has been so watched by her parents 
that she feels herself going crazy she has to tell them to the minute where 
she goes and what she does." Not only does the dominance increase in scope 
and intensity in time, but there are changes in the reason for dominance. In 
the first story the mother is dominant because of the hope she cherished for 
her son. She makes him practice "because she hopes that someday he'll be 
a great musician," although she whips him because the violin was a "genuine 
Stradivarius." There is no dominance for the sake of dominance here. The 
motivation behind the dominance in the second story is similar. "She is 
heartbroken and refuses to allow him to bring home the girl" again because 
she was "the town's bad girl," presumably frustrating her aspirations for her 
son. Although in part responsible for her son's suicide she is able to realize 
her failing and "brings home the wife and makes a lady out of her." In this 
story the mother, again, is not pictured as someone who is domineering for 



THE REGION OF THE FAMILY 14! 

the sake of domineering. In the third story, however, the parents, and this 
time it is not the mother but both parents, are presented as unreasonably 
domineering and with no other motive evident. 

2. Changes in the Child's Reaction to Dominance 

The child's reaction to this dominance exhibits more striking changes in 
time. The most striking change appears between childhood and adolescence. 
In the first story the child is openly aggressive and "breaks the violin." He 
also does something of which the mother disapproves in the second story; 
he marries the town's bad girl. But in the third story there is no evidence 
of rebellion. Moreover, his defiance of the mother's wishes in the second 
story lacks the spontaneity of expression seen in the first. There is no men- 
tion of anger in the second story or even of hating the mother's dominance 
and, secondly, the hero is portrayed on a drunken spree which seems to be 
a condition of his defiance. It is explicitly contrasted with his normal state 
in which "he has always been dominated by his mother.'* We see here the 
beginning of inhibition of self assertion and aggression which is completed 
in the third story. 

3. Changes in the Hero's Reaction to His Own Defiance 

The hero's reaction to his own defiance has also changed. In the first 
story the hero is not at all worried about the mother's feelings; his only 
concern is the whipping he may get "then he is sorry because he knows he'll 
get whipped." In the second story, however, concomitant with the reduction 
in spontaneity and freedom of expression of his defiance, his whole concern 
is with his mother's reaction "He has always been dominated by his mother 
and is very unhappy at her anger." It is a concern with the mother's state, 
rather than the consequences of the state as it was in the first story. Had 
the logic of the first story remained unchanged, the consequences of her 
anger, forbidding him to bring the girl home, rather than his unhappiness 
at her anger as such, would have caused him to commit suicide. Further 
reactions in this chain show similar changes. In the first story he is punished 
and that is all. In the second story he invokes the mother's anger, which is 
a variety of punishment that makes him unhappy, but the reaction to this is 
self destruction. In the third story, extreme parental dominance brings about 
mental collapse, which is not a striving but the end result of complete sub- 
mission to extreme dominance. 

These changes, then, are from overt aggression against limited dominance, 
and punishment which is tolerated, to inhibited aggression against greater 
dominance and maternal anger that cannot be tolerated leading to self 



1^2 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

destruction, and finally the most extreme dominance and complete inhibition 
of aggression, complete submission and mental collapse. 

4. The Fourth Story 

The end of the third story leads naturally to the fourth. In a mental institu- 
tion away from the parents, the girl becomes normal again. In the fourth story, 
with no parent figures present, and the hero now a "man" he learns that 
"anything bad he feels can just be brought out in his paintings and he'll never 
do real wrong." We are told then that in later life aggression which could 
not be expressed towards the parents without fear of ultimate self destruction 
or insanity may be sublimated in painting. The changes delineated in these 
stories are a faithful picture of this young woman's actual development. 

THE CASE OF KAROL 

Let us consider the next three stories told by another young woman, Karol, 
in which the hero is a child and then a young adult. 

For a long time the little boy has been wishing he could have a violin. His 
parents have at last bought him one and he takes great pleasure in just 
sitting and admiring it. He dreams of the day when he will be older and 
capable of making beautiful music with it after he has practiced for a long 
while. The violin will give him pleasure and relaxation when he is older 
and has mastered it. 

Sally has always been a dreamer. When she was small it didn't matter so 
much, but now that she is 12 her mother is getting worried about her 
dreams of greatness and of doing wonderful things. Sally talks about extra- 
ordinary things she plans to do someday, and won't keep her mind on the 
things at hand, such as school. Her mother tries to divert her attention by 
reading to her and buying her dolls to play with, but though she is polite 
enough to listen and cooperate with her mother, she finds no joy in ordinary 
childish pleasures. Sally's father realizes that her dreams may be turned 
into something practical, so he encourages her to become a nurse, which Sally 
realizes is a truly great profession. She is willing to study when she grows 
older with such an aim in view. 

Since Peg was a little girl she had been babied by her mother so that when 
she grew up to be 21 she was dependent on her mother for advice on 
everything. Peg's father had little to say in the matter, for like most fathers, 
he was "too busy." After finishing her education, Peg decided she would 
just stay home and look after her mother. Here is where the father came in. 
One evening the family was seated in the living room, and the subject of 
Peg's future came up. The mother said she wanted her daughter to stay 
home and be domestic. The father walked over to the couch where mother 
and daughter were sitting, and said to Peg, "What you need is a good 
spanking to make you wake up to all the opportunities open to girls these 



THE REGION OF THE FAMILY 143 

days." This speech shocked her mother, but it set Peg to thinking. The 
more she thought about it, the more she wanted to get out and see what she 
could do. In spite of her mother she did manage to get out of her over- 
solicitous control and she got a job in which she did very well. 

I. Changes in the Characterization of the Parents 

a) First story. Let us first consider the changes in the characterization of 
the parents as the individual grows from childhood to maturity. When the 
hero is a little boy, in the first story, the parents are not differentiated into 
mother and father. They are "his parents." Their impact on the child's life 
is a limited one. They neither instigate his wishes nor attempt to control them 
in any way. Ministering to the wishes of the child is their sole function. 
This however is not done immediately. The child has wished for the violin 
a long time and it is not until this wish has endured a "long time" that the 
parents "at last" bought him one. But having given the child what he wanted 
they play no further role in his life. 

b) Second story. In adolescence, as portrayed in the second story, the role 
of the parents has changed considerably. Not only have they been differ- 
entiated into mother and father, but each parent exerts a very different influ- 
ence on the adolescent's life. Parental impact on the adolescent is much 
greater than it was in childhood. The continuation into adolescence of childish 
characteristics is the explanation for this characteristics which "didn't matter 
so much" when she was small and which as we have also seen in the first story 
were unnoticed by the parents at that time. But these now mobilize anxiety 
on the part of the mother and force her to play a more active role toward 
her daughter. The mother tries to divert her attention but is unsuccessful 
because she doesn't really understand her daughter. This lack of under- 
standing was not explicit in childhood but might have been inferred from 
the role attributed to the parents in the first story. But the father has come, 
in adolescence, to play a new role. This may be explained in part by the 
influence of puberty, since at that time this young woman saw her father, 
for the first time, to be a "man," as she expressed it in her autobiography. 
The father is seen as less anxious and more intelligent in his insight; he 
encourages her to turn her energies into those channels which will provide 
the conditions necessary for her further development. 

c) Third story. In the third story, the contemporary picture of the role 
of the mother and father is presented. This portrayal presents a difficult 
problem in interpretation. The mother is said to have "babied" her daughter 
since she was a little girl. Actually this is not the case. The historical sequence 
presented by the first two stories represents the actual course of events. Why 
then, in this story dealing with the contemporary situation, has this sequence, 
mirrored faithfully to this point, suffered such restructuration? Our hypothesis 



144 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

is that this represents a restructuration which is the consequence of her present 
wish for independence, in the manner in which someone who has just fought 
witn a friend may convince himself that the person now detested was never 
a true friend, that he never really liked him. We have seen before that this 
process of restructuration of the past in terms of the present situation may 
reach back and distort characteristics of the past as they appear in TAT stories. 
The process in this case has not spread to those stories in which the picture 
presents a child, but it has influenced those stories in which the figures in 
the picture represent a contemporary situation. This, in short, is her present 
view of her past history. Craving independence she feels her relationship 
with her mother is too dependent, that her mother stifles her and, further, 
that this has always been so. The role of the father has also changed some- 
what. He is still the more intelligent of the parents, but now the opposition 
between the parents has become clearer and more profound. The father not 
only understands his daughter better and is more effective in coping with this 
daughter's problems but he appears to have a less selfish attitude toward her 
future. The mother "wanted her daughter to stay home and be domestic," 
and to continue her complete dependence. But in adolescence the mother was 
"worried" about the child and, though not understanding, tried to remedy 
the arrested development of her childish adolescent daughter. Today, how- 
ever, the mother is seen to be not only responsible for this arrested develop- 
ment but insistent that her daughter continue in this state of dependence. 
The father's intervention in adolescence was more successful than the mother's 
attempts but not in contradiction with the mother's ultimate purpose. Both 
parents were equally worried about her childlike characteristics. But in the 
contemporary situation the father's intervention "shocked her mother/' These 
differences in part reflect actual growing opposition between the parents but 
they are also derived in part from the fact that she now faces the prospect 
of cutting the umbilical cord and leaving home and feels her father to be 
more interested than her mother in her ultimate well-being. 

2. Changes in the Personality of the Hero 

a) First story* Let us consider the changes in the personality of these 
heroes. The child is full of longing directed not toward the parents but 
toward the violin. He is dependent on the parents only in so far as they 
are instrumental in securing for him what he lacks. But in this respect he 
is the passive personality who waits for the action of others to satisfy his 
needs. When granted the object of his wishes, his reaction is on the perceptual 
level "sitting and admiring it" an aesthetic appreciation rather than an 
active manipulation. His activity is limited in effectiveness by his years. 
Turning to the level of irreality and daydream he projects into the future 



THE REGION OF THE FAMILY 345 

because of his lack of competence as a child. Achievement is thought to be 
the exclusive possession of the adult or at least the older person. But as a child 
he possessed the realistic awareness that means-end activity was a necessary 
prelude to competence and enjoyment of his potentialities "after he has prac- 
ticed for a long while." Thus "he dreams of the day when he will be both 
older and capable of making beautiful music with it." But he recognizes 
that age and capability are not sufficient conditions of mastery, for these are 
qualified by "after he has practiced for a long while." Thus, although the 
hero as a child is passive, requiring the activity of the parent on his behalf, 
and although there is an aesthetic response rather than active manipulation and 
a turning to the level of daydreaming rather than behavior, he yet envisions 
a change in his future potentialities and competence. He will work hard 
when he gets older, and by virtue of both his age and his industry he will be 
"capable of making beautiful music." But the evaluation of this future 
achievement contains no social referent; neither the parent nor humanity in 
general inspire this future effort nor do they profit from it. It remains a 
solipsistic venture yielding the future adult "pleasure and relaxation." The 
achievement of "making beautiful music" is valued for no more than might 
be achieved through masturbation. It is in fact not uncommon for the playing 
of the violin to symbolize sexual experience. We lack sufficient evidence to 
know whether it is true in this case. 

b) Second story. In adolescence the hero is still a dreamer and "has always 
been a dreamer," but it is now symptomatic of arrested development. Puberty 
did, in fact, intensify her cathexis for the level of irreality. This is, of course, 
not uncommon in adolescence, but since this adolescent characteristic was 
superimposed on a long established habit of daydreaming, the resultant was a 
withdrawal more marked than is customary at this time. The content of the 
daydream has become more k extravagant and less realistic. It is now 
"extraordinary things" which she "plans to do someday." Missing is the 
expectation of hard work and the knowledge of the means-end relationship. 
Achievement is still put off into the future but there is a change in her 
communicativeness; she now "talks" about it to others. But, the most 
important change is in the fact that the daydreaming now has socially 
conspicuous consequences "she won't keep her mind on the things at 
hand, such as school." There is more overt interaction with her mother 
"she is polite enough to listen and cooperate with her mother" but this 
is a very superficial interaction for "she finds no joy in ordinary childish 
pleasures." The wish to enjoy adult pleasures which we saw in her 
childhood has continued into adolescence but the path towards the fclfill- 
ment of this wish is less certain and less realistic. It is only through her 
father's intervention that she is restored to the former confidence in her own 



146 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

future and to her previous understanding that means-end activity is neces- 
sary for achievement. Finally, she regains her willingness to work "when 
she grows older." We see that even as an adolescent she considers herself 
incapable of beginning the work which she feels is necessary to achieve her 
goal. Since the goal is an adult one and not a "childish pleasure" she cannot 
begin to strive till she is an adult. The intervention of the father has 
however effected an important difference in this goal. As a child, goal 
achievement was measured in terms of personal "pleasure and relaxation." 
This aspiration has been socialized by the father and she now realizes that 
nursing "is a truly great profession." If a sexual symbolism is involved one 
might suppose that the solitary pleasure of masturbation has been transformed 
into a sexual aim with a human object, through the agency of puberty and 
new interest between father and daughter. But whether or not this is the 
case, the father has been instrumental in turning the adolescent dream towards 
social reality. 

c.) Third story. The daughter as a young adult of 21 is a somewhat 
different person. We have previously discussed the reasons for this. In part 
it is the consequence of the hypertrophied level of irreality and daydream. 
The daydream which inspired her childhood was somewhat responsible for 
the arrest of development at adolescence but continuing into adulthood it has 
produced a 21 year old "baby," "dependent on her mother for advice on 
everything." The heroine attributes this to the fact that "since Peg was a 
little girl she had been babied by her mother." It is not attributed, as it 
should be, and as it was before to her own daydreaming, but to her mother 
the same mother who became worried about her daughter for the first time 
in adolescence and who though incompetent meant to help her daughter escape 
from infantilism. Although this is her present picture of how her mother 
has always behaved, we have seen that this restructuration is something 
which the individual does not altogether believe is true otherwise the actual 
historical sequence could not have been projected so faithfully in her first 
two stories. In those cases in which the past is completely restructured there 
is no evidence in the backward projections of the actual historical develop- 
ment of the child-parent relaionship. Evidence from other sources indicates 
that in all probability she pictures her mother as responsible for her infantilism 
because it is now completely unacceptable to her, having reached the age of 
adulthood, when according to her childhood dream she should have been 
"making beautiful music" and when according to her adolescent dream she 
should have been "willing to study" with "an aim in view." The mother 
has in effect become a scapegoat, to relieve the daughter of the unacceptable 
consequences resulting from an excessive immersion in phantasy. But in 
addition to this, she has been drawing closer and closer to her father and 



THE REGION OF THE FAMILY 147 

in her mind this involves rejection of the mother on the part of both father 
and daughter. The father's suggestions "shocked the mother," but in 
adolescence the same suggestion had no such effect, since the mother shared 
the father's interest in helping the daughter establish realistic social contact 
with her environment. There has been throughout the three stories an 
identification with the father, which is implicit in the first story, openly 
avowed in adolescence, and culminates in opposition between father, 
daughter, and mother in adulthood. Evidence suggestive of the family 
romance was also found in the other stories and autobiography. The third 
story tells us in effect that only as a young adult did she face this conflict 
openly. As a child there was the dream of playing an adult role. As an 
adolescent the father sustained the dream, and today her father will help 
her achieve the dream against the opposition of her mother. Though it 
shocks the mother and "in spite of her mother," she did manage to get out 
of her oversolicitous control. With her father's help, openly breaking with 
her mother, the adult heroine, just come of age, "got a job in which she 
did very well." This is the first reference to actual achievement. It is no 
longer placed in the future. This story represents a very recent change in 
her orientation. She does in fact plan in the very near future to follow the 
example of her heroine. It is noteworthy that the heroine is 21 years old, 
whereas Karol is not quite 21. 

THE CASE OF LANS 

The following three stories show the developmental sequence of an 
individual's increasing hostility towards his parents. 

Little Johnny looked forward to a pleasant day outside playing baseball with 
his pals. Before he got half way to the front door, mother grabbed him, 
and led him by the arm into the music room, and sat him down in front of 
a violin. "Practice your lesson for an hour or you can't go out to play 
today." Johnny sat and looked tearfully at the violin. What will the outcome 
be? Very simple he will practice for an hour on the violin. 

They were seated around the dinner table. Father and son were violently 
arguing. Father was very angry. Junior's grades were not what they should 
be. Father sent Junior from the table when Junior remarked jon father's 
grades during the days of his youth. Junior retired to his room angrily, and 
finally was practically dissolved into tears when he thought of the injustice of 
it all. He was hungry too. After a while, father knocked on the door and 
came in. He put his arm around the boy, and Junior leaned on his shoulder 
feeling exceedingly sorry for himself. Father did not scold. He talked in a 
quiet voice and told the boy to please try to work harder in school. Junior 
relented and promised. He actually believed that he would. And at first he 
did, but gradually lapsed back into laziness again. 

Joel came in the door. He was mad. His father could see that very plainly. 
The usual chip was on his should. He must have a talk with Joel. He 



1 48 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

must learn to be more even tempered, and not fly off at any small thing. The 
father said "Joel come here son, I want to talk to you." Joel said, "I know 
what you're going to say father, but I can't do anything. Please father, ifs 
no use. Some other time** and left the room. 

Changes in the Expression of Aggression 

We have seen before that aggression, freely expressed in childhood, may 
later sutler severe inhibition. In these stories the opposite developmental 
sequence is illustrated. In the first story the hero's reaction to parental 
dominance is one of tearful compliance. In the second story paternal 
dominance evokes anger but the hero retires to his room at his father's 
insistence. There is again a regression to the original response. He "finally 
practically dissolved into tears when he thought of the injustice of it all," 
The father then comes in and puts his arm around the boy, and as a result the 
boy determines to work harder in school although he "gradually lapsed back 
into laziness again.'* That this individual should be divided within himself 
between anger and tears is probably a result of his father's oscillation between 
stern dominance and sympathetic nurturance. In the third story, however, 
the hero has come to terms with his conflict. He is now capable of expressing 
more unambivalent aggression: "He was mad. His father could see that 
very plainly. The usual chip was on his shoulder. He must have a talk with 
Joel. He must learn to be more even tempered, and not fly off at any small 
thing." In response to his father's dominance he "left the room" but not 
to dissolve in tears. The hero has accepted his own personality with all 
its limitations and will not allow himself to be swayed by his father. "I know 
what you're going to say father, but I can't do anything. Please father, it's 
no use. Some other time and he left the room." 



THE CASE OF BRINT 



In the following two stories there is a delineation of the development 
of an ego ideal. 

Joe has been given one of his greatest wishes. A violin. He had, during his 
short life, dreamt of becoming a great violinist. Strange, he never knew why, 
but he did. But now that he had received this precious gift he realized his 
future task would not be as easy as his dream. It meant work, hard work, 
long hours of toil and probably agony for everyone else, but he was still 
very young, and his mind was set and his heart and mind were as determined 
as his face. And he'll reach his goal; I feel sure of that! 

Ifs only natural for a son to want to follow in his father's footsteps or is 
it? Well, in the case of young Dick it was so. His father was a surgeon and 
Dick had watched many operations. His mother didn't approve of that but 



THE REGION OF THE FAMILY 149 

she did wish her son to follow his father. Dick didn't care about being 
famous, he just wanted to help people and the sooner he could get to it 
the better. He was still very young, but he could do it, he would do it and 
he did! 

The hero in both stories is driven by an ideal which determines his life. 
It is of little moment that one of these wishes is to be a violinist and one to 
be a surgeon. These differences may be attributed to the fact that the first 
story was told to the picture of the boy and the violin, and the second to the 
operation scene. But what is of import is the difference in aim and in 
awareness of the origin of the ego ideal. In the first story the child does not 
know: "Strange, he never knew why but he did." But in the second story he is 
clearly aware of it's origin, "It's only natural for a son to want to follow in 
his father's footsteps or is it? Well, in the case of young Dick it was so." 
Although aware of this origin, the remarks of the subject "or is it?" 
suggest some question of the naturalness of this aspiration. A young woman 
told these stories and the fact that "his mother didn't approve of that" may 
indicate anxiety about identification with the father against the mother's 
wishes and may further explain why the younger boy did not understand 
the origin of his dream of becoming a great violinist. Whatever the mean- 
ing, it is clear that there has been an important change in both awareness of 
the origin of his dream and in its direction, "he just wanted to help people 
and the sooner he could get to it the better." He is no longer interested as 
he was in his career as a violinist in "being famous." The ideal of becoming 
a great violinist had a less social meaning; there was no mention either of an 
audience or of giving pleasure through his playing or pleasing his parents. 

There is also an increased confidence that the goal may be attained. In 
the first story success is placed in the future and the storyteller is "sure" he'll 
reach his goal: "He'll reach his goal, I feel sure of that!" But in the second 
story success is actually achieved in the future: "He was still very young, but 
he could do it, nc would do it, and he did!" These differences again reflect 
the importance of the avowed identification with the father as the hero 
matures. 

THE CASE OF FRANK 

The following two stories are representative of a not uncommon sequence 
concerning the emancipation of the child from parental ego idealism. 

The little boy has just been told that he must practice on his violin and is 
looking at it with hatred for he knows there is a baseball game going on 
outside. After a while he will pick up the violin and practice without any 
thought of what he is doing but just go through the motions of it without 
any thought 

The young man's mother had planned on her son becoming a great physician 
as jus father had been. After going through pre-medical school ne realizes 



150 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

that he will never make a good surgeon for he cannot bear to see others in 
pain. He knows that he will be a great baseball player if he wants to 
devote his energy to it. He tells his mother of his great chance to become a 
member of a professional team. When he breaks the news to her, her 
dreams arc shattered. But realizing that her son is at an age where he must 
make his own decisions, she consents. It of course hurts the .son to grieve 
his mother but he must go ahead with the work he loves to become a 
great hero on the baseball diamond. Later the mother is to find joy in 
seeing her son being cheered by millions. 

The mother is a dominant figure in both stories. In the first story, "The 
litde boy has just been told that he must practice on his violin/' and in the 
second story, "The young man's mother had planned on her son becoming 
a great physician as his father had been." As a litde boy he pays lip service 
to the maternal dictate "go through the motions of it without any thought." 
He "is looking at it with hatred" however because there is a baseball game 
going on outside. In the second story he realizes that the career planned 
for him by his mother is not for him. There is neither "hatred** nor 
mechanical compliance. Although it shatters the mother's dreams, he tells 
her of his plans to become a professional baseball player, which is what his 
mother would not allow him to do when he was younger. He eventually 
not only achieves his goal but proves that he was right and his mother 
wrong in her plans. "Later the mother is to find joy in seeing her son being 
cheered by millions." His mechanical compliance with maternal dominance 
in early childhood has been transformed into an assertion of his own 
individuality and a realization of the potentialities previously so misunderstood 
by his mother. 

In these few samples of temporal sequences we have seen that quite apart 
from later changes in personality which are a resultant of the diminution of 
interaction with parents there may be marked changes in attitude o both 
child and parent within the family setting as they adjust to eacL other's 
increasing age. Stable inflexible parent-child relationships there are, but 
development is the typical characteristic of the maturing child and may 
temper the rigidity of the parents' later years. 

SUMMARY 

We have attempted to provide criteria useful in the assessment of the 
relative importance of the family region for the individual. Our purpose 
was twofold: first, to provide techniques for determining the relative 
importance of any region, whether it be the region of work, family, love, 
or social relationships, and second, to examine the relationship between child- 
hood and the adult personality. We found that the importance of the family 



THE REGION OF THE FAMILY 151 

might be indicated by direct references in die stories, but that because this 
was uncommon more indirect techniques had to be employed. Such tech- 
niques included consideration of the introduction of parental figures, the 
interpretation of ambiguous figures as parental figures, the interpretation of 
older figures as parental figures, the omission of older adults in the picture, 
the number of stories about the family, the length of such stories, and the 
intensity of their affect. More important was the criterion of the degree of 
conflict with other regions. The importance of the family may be such that 
no conflict between the family and other regions is possible; the individual 
is completely absorbed by the family or the family may mean so little that 
no conflict is involved. The individual may leave home with no conflict and 
find his chief values elsewhere but where there is a conflict the outcome may 
provide a measure of the relative importance of those regions which have 
created divided loyalties. We examined the extent to which the parents 
influenced the child within the family setting. There were found to be 
parents who through either indifference, incompetence, or complete submis- 
sion to the child's demands exerted relatively little influence on the formation 
of his personality. There were also parents whose influence was so per- 
vasive that the child's personality bore the heavy impress of the parental 
mold. We also examined the subtle interplay and mutual adaptation which 
may occur between parent and child, each influencing to some extent the 
personality of the other. We found it illuminating to examine the scope of 
parental intervention in other regions; the parent may intervene in every- 
thing which concerns the child within and outside the family or limit such 
intervention to one particular region, such as the choice of a career or wife. 
We next considered the duration of parental impact, and found that a child 
may reject parental influence very early or continue to depend on parental 
help throughout his life. These differences could be attributed either to the 
child or the parent. The parent might continue intervention despite rejec- 
tion by the child or in response to appeals for parental help. We also 
examined the generalization of the parent-child relationship, the extent to 
which the individual casts others into a parental role and reacts to them as 
if they were his parents. We found evidence of varying degrees of differen- 
tiation between the child's reaction to his parents and his later marital, work, 
and social relationships. Finally, we examined the changes in the nature o 
the parent-child relationship as the child developed into maturity. We found 
the TAT to reflect faithfully actual developmental sequences. We found, 
however, two types of distortion of this projection into the past, which 
necessitated caution in the use of the TAT for diagnosis of the individual's 
development. First we showed that events occurring in adolescence might Ac 
telescoped and attributed to the hero's childhood. Secondly we found that 



152 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

contemporary problems may be projected back into the past, so that all the 
heroes struggle with the same problem that concerns the adult who is telling 
these stories, In those protocols which are not modified by these two types 
of distortion, we found marked changes in attitudes of both child and parent 
within the family setting as they adjusted to each other's increasing age. We 
saw that development is the typical characteristic of the maturing child and 
that it not infrequently tempered the rigidity of the parents* later years. 



CHAPTER VII 

DIAGNOSIS OF PERSONALITY: THE REGION OF LOVE, 
SEX, AND MARITAL RELATIONSHIPS 

A knowledge of the individual's adjustment in the love, marital, and 
sex region is necessary to an understanding of his personality. Aspects of 
personality not revealed in more general social relationships may be revealed 
in love relationships. Moreover, discrepancies between this and other regions 
often shed considerable light on personality structure. Adjustment in this 
area may be crucial to the individual's total adjustment. 

In part, the individual's expectations, wishes, and fears in a love relation- 
ship bear the imprint of the family, as we have seen, but the two regions 
are sufficiently different to justify further analysis. Let us consider those 
dimensions of the love relationship crucial to the diagnosis of an individual's 
adjustment in this area. 

I. DIMENSIONS OF THE LOVE AND MARITAL REGION 
A. THE NATURE OF THE RELATIONSHIP 

I. THE DEFINITION OF THE SITUATION 

Of primary importance is the individual's definition of the nature of the 
love relationship. What role does the individual wish or expect to play; 
what does he expect from his love object? Is there a discrepancy between 
the role he plays and his ideal role? The stories which follow are a sample 
of the more commonly expressed phantasies. Perhaps one of the most 
common definitions of the love relationship is seen in the first story, in which 
love is defined in terms of gratification of dependency wishes (vector "on"). 

This is a young girl who has been waiting for the right man to come 
along. She has never liked boys her own age because they don't understand 
her. She wants someone older that she can talk to, someone she can. rely 
on to stand by her and take care of her, someone that she can depend on. 
Then she went to work for Mr, Rogers and in the course of working together 
they discovered that they had a lot in common. They spent long hours 
talking together and finally woke up to the fact that they had fallen in love. 
She is ecstatically happy because at kst she has found the man she had 
dreamed about 

We see at the beginning of the story a discrepancy between her actual 
role and her ideal role which is remedied when she meets "the man she had 
dreamed about.*' 



154 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

These wishes not infrequently are overlaid with a fear of loss of respect. 

This woman is blissfully happy because she had found someone who returns 
her affection with equal intensity. The man here is a little amused at her 
childish clinging because he had thought her so mature and self-contained. 
She on her part is happy because she can cling to him as much as she likes 
without fear of his ceasing to respect her on that score; she wants to feel 
thus protected and cherished always so that she might in turn be a source 
of strength and faith. 

Through the gratification of dependency without fear of loss of respect, 
the heroine of this story is enabled in turn to "be a source of strength and 
faith" (vector "for"), whereas the gratification of dependency in the previous 
story was an end in itself. 

This wish to help the love object (vector "for") appears in purer torm 
in the following story. 

All she asks of life is a chance to make a home for him and to take care of 
him. She pictures herself waking him in the morning, fixing his breakfast 
and seeing him off to work and then spending the day doing things in prepara- 
tion for his return. That is the way marriage should be, she thought 

Here the wish to be of help to the love object is as unfettered as was the 
wish to be helped in the first story. 

A second definition of the love relationship is in terms either of govern- 
ing or being governed (vectors "over" and "by"). In the following story the 
heroine governs her love object. 

She is the kind of woman who has a strong influence over her husband. She 
believes that she knows best about everything and she has convinced him 
of this. She loves him but she just can't let him make a decision by himself. 
This has been going on for so many years that he has given up any idea of 
trying to change it 

The heroine of the next story is submissive, governed completely by the 
wishes of her love object. 

Well, anyhow here is a very timid woman. She has the kind of husband 
who's very overpowering, a very strong character. She spends her whole 
life trying to please him. She likes to have things the way he wants them and 
whenever he tells her to do anything she hurries to do it. She wants very 
much to please him and he's her whole life. 

These needs are complementary as were the needs to depend on or take 
care of the love object. Conflict, frustration, and an unstable relationship 
may result if an individual having one of these needs enters into a relation- 
ship with someone whose needs are not complementary. We see this in the 



LOVE, SEX, AND MARITAL RELATIONSHIPS 155 

following story in which the man's wish to care for the woman is frustrated 
by her self sufficiency. 

She had everything she wants, and doesn't take him seriously, and he 
wishes there were something he could do for her that would make her 
conscious of him. He wants to take care o her, but she's so self sufficient 
that he doesn't ever have a chance. One afternoon when she's been out 
riding he has his chance. She's become very tired from riding and he takes 
her to his little house that's out in the field. He tells her to go to sleep and 
she lies down and he comes over and tells her a story, and sh,e reacts very 
well. In fact she goes to sleep while he's talking. While she's asleep he's 
making elaborate plans to get her something to eat He gets exotic foods 
around the house, cheeses and wines, etc., and he plans a very happy 
evening together, but suddenly she wakes up and realizes that it's late, and 
so she rides off and says good-bye. So he very sadly put away all the prepara- 
tions and yet he thinks it's nice that she had been sleeping there while he 
fixed dinner, and that for a little while he had been able to feel that she 
was his and depended on him, 

A third type of love relationship involves neither nurturance, dependence, 
dominance, nor submission, but a sharing of experience (vector "with") as its 
essential characteristic. 

Margaret and John have been married for three years and are a wonderful 
couple. They enjoy going places together, sitting home reading, listening 
to records on the Capehart, talking things over together and lead a very 
happy life. 

A fourth definition of the love relationship is in terms of acquiring and 
possessing a love object (vector "from"). 

He married her because she was the most beautiful thing he had ever seen 
and he desired to possess her as he might possess any other beautiful thing. 
He never let her out of his sight He had to know that she was always 
there and that she was his. 

A fifth variety is based on the aesthetic enjoyment of the love object 
(vector "toward")* 

As he looked at her he marveled at how lightly the years had touched her. 
They had been married for fifteen years and every time he looked at her 
the miracle of her beauty was a new discovery. Her deep brown eyes had 
softened a little and her hair had been touched with gray, but she still had 
the same proud grace that had made him fall in love with her. 

Although such direct references to the nature of the definition of the 
love relationship are not infrequent, there are protocols which require more 
indirect techniques to elicit this material. There may be stories in which only 
the threat to a love relationship is elaborated, but implicit in the threat is the 
meaning of the love relationship. 



156 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

Thus, in the following story, the threat of disruption of the love relation- 
ship is essentially a threat to the hero's need to govern the behavior of his 
love object. 

She has just told him that she's going to leave him. She said it calmly and 
quietly as though it were an everyday happening. He can't believe his ears. 
She's always been a model wife, so different than other guy's wives. At 
first he thinks she's kidding but then he realizes she isn't Then he thinks 
there's another man, but she says she just wants to live by herself and run 
her own life. He can't understand it, but he's sure she'll come back. 

2. THE RECIPROCITY OF LOVE 

Another important dimension is the degree of reciprocity in the love 
relationship. Does the individual see his love returned in equal measure? 
The following three stories represent the middle and end points of a con- 
tinuum ranging from complete mutuality to complete lack of reciprocity, 
Love in the first story is completely mutual. 

These are two people probably married for some short period of time who, 
at the point of this picture, had made one small step further in reaching a 
fundamental understanding of each other. They are now both content with 
full realization of their deep love for each other. They are both fairly 
intelligent people and their deep desires to establish complete mental harmony 
are the controlling factors in their entire life. Their marriage is a happy 
one and the ability to overlook each other's defects and to appreciate each 
other's qualities enable them to lead a very full life. 

In the next story, however, mutuality is achieved; at the beginning of 
the story the heroine "isn't too interested in him yet." But her attitude 
changes and "they will make a very interesting and happily married couple." 

She isn't too interested in him yet but he has decided that she is the girl that 
that he will make a play for not just a play for he really has his eye on 
her .... She is still sore as far as the subject of men is concerned and has a 
rather self-sufEcient hands-off-me attitude but she just can't put this man in 
his place, he is too smart and she realizes, not without some hostility and 
ambivalence that he is a man she will have to reckon with. After he slapped 
her down at the right times and showed tenderness at the appropriate times 
they will make a very interesting and happily married couple. 

In the third story, the end point of the continuum, we see a complete 
lack of reciprocity. 

Well, I think he's going to war. She says she wants hirr> to go. "You are 
an American she says and I want you to fight for your county and die for 
it," she says. She's bidding him farewell. "I must tell her before I go," he 
says, "but I don't know how." There's someone else in my life. "She's in 
love with me but I'm not in love with her." You see she's in love with him 



LOVE, SEX, AND MARITAL RELATIONSHIPS 157 

but he's not in love with her. "I don't believe in marriage without love," 
he says. "She's in love with me but I'm not in love with her and I must 
*ell her there's someone else." And she's saying, "I want you to die a noble 
and honorable death in the service of your country because I know you 
don't love me." She wants nobody else to have him if she can't 

3. THE INTENSITY OF LOVE 

The intensity of the feeling of love is independent of the nature of the 
love relationship and the extent to which love is reciprocated. These three 
itories represent the middle and end points of the scale. 

This man has lost his wife to his best friend. He is so terribly in love with 
his wife that he looks only for her happiness and gives her to his best friend. 

This woman has led an ordinary life. She has a happy family, probably two 
or three children. And she has a business-like husband whom she respects 
deeply but never passionately loved. At the point of this picture this is late 
at night her family's in bed she has just finished reading. She's going 
around turning out lights and shutting windows in preparation for bed her- 
self. Her life will continue as it has been before. 

Camilla was married to John. It was a forced marriage sort of a family 
wish, and even though they didn't love each other there was no way of 
getting out of it 

4. TEMPORAL CHARACTERISTICS 

a) Maturation 

Another independent dimension of the love relationship is its temporal 
characteristic. The maturation of the relationship should be examined. Does 
the hero speak of falling in love at first sight, as in the following story, or 
does he describe a long acquaintance which gradually ripens into love? 

It was at a hotel in New York at which this handsome young man planned 
to stay. There was also a beautiful woman, who stayed also. Both, came 
down to breakfast at the same time. Suddenly, they apparently were in love 
at first sight They couldn't believe it, but it was so, so true that they began 
to plan their marriage. This doesn't seem practical, but yet it was probable. 
They fixed their wedding day October 25, 1829 and planned to be married 
by that day. They couldn't, believe that those two were to be together. The 
night before the wedding they were all excited, too excited to tfr*"k of any- 
thing else. As the time passed they sat silently waiting for the hour, the 
minute, the second for them to be united as one. 

That love may not be so spontaneous a phenomenon is laboriously 
elaborated in the following story told by a cautious young woman. 

Jill had lived a most unexciting life; she had gone to school and then started 
working at the 5 and dime store which was more monotonous tfran it was 



158 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

exciting. One day a strange man came in the store and bought a bag of 
candy which was' where Jill worked. Two days later he came back and 
bought another bag of candy but this time he stood and stared at her for 
quite some time. On the third day, he came back again and bought another 
bag of the same cheap stuff and then he began to make conversation with 
Jill by asking her her name. He finally got around to asking Jill if she 
would pose for a picture he was going to paint. He knew that this was a 
most unusual way of getting a model but after seeing her the first day he 
was in he knew that she had the type of face that he wanted to use in his 
new work. He assured her that everything would be on the up and up and 
that she could come nights and sit as he too had a job in the day time. At 
first she was a little afraid but then after the first night she knew that he 
was really on the level and in spite of everything he said he was really a 
good painter, but then she was no judge of these things. There was one 
thing that Jill didn't count on and that was her falling in love with Chris 
which she did in a very short time but it was only natural for they were 
together every night for such a long time and he was the first man who had 
ever paid any attention to her in her life with the exception of a boy with 
red hair back in the tenth grade who used to take her out and buy her ice 
cream sodas. Jill was excited by this new thing that had happened to her 
and for her life had taken a new turn. When the picture was finished she 
suddenly realized that everything was over and that again she must go back 
to the same type of life she had been living, store, home to dinner, and be 
back to the store again. Jill was determined that this was not going to 
happen to her for after all she had spent so much time trying to make some- 
thing of herself, but little did she know that Chris had fallen in love with 
her also and that soon again he was going to ask her to pose for another 
picture so that he could be near her. Both of them were too proud to come 
out and tell the other that he was in love with the other so time passed, two 
years to be exact, and the war had been declared and Chris was leaving for the 
camouflage department before he told Jill of his love for her and asked 
her to wait for him. 

Not infrequently, the rate of maturation of the love relationship may 
differ between the lovers. 

The man has just asked the girl to marry him. Uh, she is very much in 
love with him and although he doesn't know it, the minute she saw him she 
decided that he was hers. She started an intricate campaign and ran away 
from him, uh so much that he became interested. 

b) Duration 

Duration is another temporal characteristic of the love relationship which 
must be considered. How long does the relationship endure? In lie story 
which follows, the relationship endures for only a short time; one of the 
lovers dies "in her teens." 

When a young boy, Roger Rollins brought his girl, Mary Caudry, to this 
tree and carved their initials on it Later in her "teens," Mary died; and 
Roger every year makes a pilgrimage to this tree. 



LOVE, SEX, AND MARITAL RELATIONSHIPS 159 

That the relationship may be long-enduring is seen in the next story in 
which a couple are celebrating their fiftieth wedding anniversary. 

Pictured here are a man and wife on their fiftieth wedding anniversary. 
Their children and grandchildren have made them a surprise wedding anni- 
versary party. When the evening draws to a close, they retire to their room 
and start to reminisce. They remember all the years they have spent together. 
They think of the troubles they have gone through, the sacrifices they have 
made for one another and their children and also the many happy moments 
they have shared together. They admit to one another there have been times 
when they wondered if perhaps they would have been happier if they hadn't 
married. But when they think of all the wonderful years they have spent 
together, they realize that theirs has been the perfect marriage. 

Fine gradations in the portrayal of the duration of the love relationship 
are found between these end points. In the following story for example, we 
see a love relationship which is neither short-lived nor long-enduring. 

Man is deeply in love with a woman. He has come to tell her that their 
love cannot endure due to extenuating circumstances. She is sobbing and 
he is deeply moved. They both appear strong. She releases him in spite of 
her great love for him. He tries but never is able to get over the great 
love of his life. 

These stories reflect differences in the duration of the love relationship; 
we must, however, also inquire into the duration of the -feelings of love. The 
relationship may oudast the feelings of love; the feelings may continue after 
the relationship is broken; the feelings may continue only as long as the 
relationship is maintained; or the relationship may be maintained only as 
long as the feelings continue. 

Following is a story in which the individual breaks of! the relationship 
at the moment his feelings change. 

He wasn't very capable of loving anyone for very long. He fell in and out 
of love as easily as a fish swimming in and out of the waves, and Anne was 
no exception. He loved her passionately, but not for long, and she couldn't 
hold him. 

Or the duration of the feelings may be a function of the maintenance of 
the love relationship; when this is broken the feelings change. 

This woman ias a weakness for men. She's rather oversexed but she is a 
woman with lots of style, rather sensuous and quite wealthy. One day out 
in the country she was riding, and she came to an open field where she 
saw a mao working. So she went to talk to the man just because she thought 
it would be fun and that he would naturally be thrilled to talk to her. 
Before very long, a couple of weeks, they were in love. He doesn't love her 
quite the way she loves him. She wants to marry him. He likes to have 



l6o THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

fun with women. At this point, he has just told her that he can't tie him- 
self down. This hurts, but she decides that she won't cry over any man, and 
she goes back to her old way of life and falls out of love quite easily. 

Feelings may continue for varying periods of time after the relationship 
has been broken. The heroine of the following story adjusts in a relatively 
short time to the broken relationship and "begins a new life for herself." 

I'd say that this girl had just received a telegram from the government 
telling her that her husband had been killed in action. She doesn't know how 
she'll be able to live without him because she has centered her whole life 
about him; but, because she is still young, she later becomes adjusted to the 
fact that he won't come back and, uh, begins a new life for herseE Later 
on, she remarries and, uh, had a very happy life with her husband and 
family, and her life becomes broader since she has known grief. 

Whereas it takes the heroine of the next story "a long, long time to 
get over it." 

This girl is crying desperately the man she loves has just been there and 
told her that the whole thing has been a mistake that he thought he loved 
her but the war has changed everything and he doesn't feel that he can 
marry her she faces a dark and desolate future all her dreams are shattered 
and at this moment she doesn't know what she'll do. She feels helpless 
because she still really doesn't know what's wrong why he changed his mind. 
She feels very alone and it'll take her a long, long time to get over it but 
eventually she'll meet someone she can trust who will always love her and 
never leave her and she'll be happy with him. 

And the heroine of the following story never recovers from the broken 
relationship; her feelings continue until she dies. 

This picture, to me, depicts an old woman, about seventy years old, peering 
with longing in her eyes. She is alone in this house in which she Hves, and 
has been alone there for the past ten years since her husband left died, I 
mean. They were a devoted couple and loved each other dearly. His death 
shocked her and since they were childless, she was left positively alone. The 
past ten years haven't at all worn off his absence. At times the sense of 
loneliness so greatly overcomes her that she begins searching the house for 
him believing he is still there, sitting and reading. This idea which she holds, 
that he is still alive and present in the house, so overwhelms her as years go 
on, that eventually, one night, she imagines seeing him in a chair and goes 
over and speaks to him. The next morning some neighbors, missing her, 
search the house and find her seated in a chair very comfortably opposite an 
empty chair. She had died of a heart attack during die night 

Less frequently do we find references to the maintenance of a love 
relationship after the feelings have changed. The following story was told 
by a disenchanted woman of 40. 



LOVE, SEX, AND MARITAL RELATIONSHIPS l6l 

These people are middle aged. They've been married for years. They were 
once in love a long time ago. But they've gone their separate ways. Nothing 
but the habit of living together through the years keeps them together. 
They're just used to each other, and will probably live together for the rest 
of their lives, going on in the same way. They're just in a rut 

A preoccupation with duration and constancy may signify more than 
the temporal characteristic of the love relationship. In such cases, the type 
of love relationship is no longer a primary consideration. The concern with 
constancy and duration supersedes the individual's more specific love needs. 
In the following example, the individual who is suffering a basic lack of self 
confidence cannot enjoy the love relationship, afraid that it will not last. 

Ever since the night he had proposed she had been afraid she couldn't hold 
him. She had lived in fear of this day. She had been the plain girl of 
their crowd and she knew people had whispered when they became engaged, 
"I wonder what he sees in her." Now she knew it was over all her worst 
fears were realized. She wouldn't put up a fight what could she do against 
a beautiful, clever girl like Jane? She was just the plain girl of the crowd 
who had married a wonderful man and had been foolish enough to hope 
against hope she could hold him. Tears streamed down her face. 

B. THE VICISSITUDES OF THE LOVE RELATIONSHIP 

I. THE SOURCE OF DISRUPTION 

The love relationship, vulnerable to diverse threats, may be threatened or 
disrupted by purely exogenous forces. 

For seven long months, Dick had been waiting for this day. At 8 pjn. 
the train was to arrive that would bring his fiancee, Jean, to Boston to be 
married that next week. Dick waited until 9 and was not too upset for 
trains are usually late these days anyway. He passed another hour or so, and 
then the news arrived that the train had been wrecked going over a washed- 
out bridge, and that a list of casualties would follow. Dick waited anxiously 
until i ajn. and word came through that Jean had been killed. Dick was 
beside himself, and being almost alone in the station he found it easy to 
overpower a shore policeman and take his gun. Dick made his way to a 
secluded section of the station waiting room, put a bullet in his head, and 
slumped onto the bench. He had no relatives and no one to care for him 
except Jean. So maybe it was the best thing to happen to a young person 
of such weak character. 

Or disruption may stem from sources partly exogenous and partly endogen- 
ous. Thus, in the following story, both dissatisfaction with the love object 
and a more desirable love object who is available contribute to the disrup- 
tion of the original relationship. 

A woman is married to* a man who has a great deal of money but whose 
other qualifications for a successful husband are nil. He is several years older 



162 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

than she, and she is still young and beautiful. It is not surprising, therefore, 
that after three years of married life she should have a lover. He comes one 
afternoon when her husband is working and has tea in one corner of the 
sumptuous living room. She is bidding him good-bye at the door when her 
husband comes home. The three of them quarrel and the lover decides to 
leave town for good. The table and teacups remain behind as mute evidence 
of a flame that died. 

The threat may, however, be primarily endogenous, stemming from dis- 
satisfactions arising completely within the relationship. 

Lydia is a perfect lady and great fun but during two years of married life 
she has not been a companion to Edward. He's a very sincere person who 
works hard, wants and expects much love from his wife and had thought 
of marriage as a happy, companionable life for two people in love who 
wanted more than anything else to share the same interests, a home, etc. He 
finally comes to the point where he tells Lydia that he isn't happy and he 
thinks it best that they "call the whole thing off" and each go his own way. 
Lydia's the type that doesn't need too much love and home life and is 
content to live as they have but she begs him to stay, promising him that 
she'll always be kind to him and bear his children, if he wants them, and 
be a good mother to them. He decides that she may grow to love him and 
that she is a good wife and that^ after all, maybe one can't expect too much. 



2. THE MAINTENANCE OR DISRUPTION OF THE RELATIONSHIP 

Let us consider now the extent to which these threats disrupt the 
relationship. 

The threat may be overcome and the relationship maintained intact. 

Mary and Jim had been engaged for three months and had their first fight 
It wasn't a very big argument but it worried each of them. Finally, because 
he was sure that it was his fault, Jim decided to go and ask Mary to forgive 
him. When the maid at Mary's house answered the door, she was sure that 
Jim had come to break his engagement to Mary at least, she gathered that 
from what Mary told her about their serious quarrel and how it was all her 
fault. When the maid saw them come in the living room a half hour later 
beaming at each other, she decided never to try to understand human beings, 
especially those in love. 

Or, the threat may not disrupt the relationship yet produce important 
changes. 

I think I have been reading too many Ladies' Home Journals. I think some 
one has told him gossip about the girl. She is at present pleading with him 
not to believe what he has heard. She is a very attractive young woman. So 
it wouldn't be difficult for people to misunderstand her actions. I believe 
he wants to believe that she is right in her denial of the gossip and yet he is 
man enough to be hurt by what he has heard. I think he will accept her 



LOVE, SEX, AND MARITAL RELATIONSHIPS 163 

side of the story but with with slight reservation. Ah a slight " reservation 
will ah cast a shadow on their future relationship because he will never 
be able to fully believe in her. 

Or the threat may completely shatter the relationship. 

Jane stood behind the tree and watched the girl in the blue evening gown 
run down the beach. Jane knew what was going to happen. She hadn't 
meant to spy. The girl ran on. A man rose up from the rocks at the other 
end of the beach. They clasped each other in their arms. Jane gasped, 
turned, and scrambled back up the rocks to the house. She didn't want 
to look. She didn't want her husband to see her. She told him their 
marriage was over when he returned. 

a) Maintenance of the Love Relationship 

If threats to the love relationship are successfully met, how is this 
achieved? 

In the following story, the relationship is maintained by the husband's 
tolerance and the heroine's new appreciation of his qualities. 

The wife came back after leaving home for a little affair. She appreciated 
her husband after she left him. He loves her and is patient and willing to 
wait. He is older and knew she was subject to these little flights. He 
expected it because she is very beautiful. He knew she really loved him. 
They live happily ever after because now she appreciates him. 

The couple may be reconciled through a common sorrow. 

The wife is resentful, of course, of the other girl and is justified. The husband 
in the meantime is drawing away from the other woman and closer to his 
wife. Her pleading and begging have made an impression in his mind 
and he realizes that his wife means more to him than anyone else. Through 
an unfortunate accident, she dies while in childbirth no, make it the baby 
who dies being born. But die two have been reunited in their grief. 

Or through reformation on the part of the hero. 

Here the impression is received that the young lady has discovered her 
lover or husband being unfaithful and is pleading with him to leave the 
girl with whom he has been conducting his infidelity. Possibly the reason 
for the lover or husband, which ever the case may be, being there is that 
hk. met the young lady of his unfaithfulness at a bar while under the 
influence of alcohol. Having been caught in the act and through his true 
love's pleading, he mends his ways. 

Or the hero may be reconciled to a lower income of love. 

He finally comes to the^ point where he tells Lydia that he isn't happy and 
he thinks it best that they "call the whole thing ofP* and each go his own 
way. Lydia's the type that doesn't need too much love and home life and is 



jg^ THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TOST 

content to live as they have but she begs him to stay, promising him that 
she'll always be kind to him and bear his children, i he wants them, and 
be a good mother to them. He decides that she may grow to love him 
and that she is a good wife and that, after all, maybe one can't expect too 
much. 

b) The Sequelae of the Disrupted Relationship 

If the love relationship suffers disruption, what are the sequelae? 
The individual may never relinquish the possibility of future 
reconciliation, 

Roger was an artist of little note and lived with his wife in a small village. 
He decided that he must go to the Nudist Colony to paint as most of his 
works were like those of many of other artists, and that he would become 
famous through this work as no one else had portrayed such a life. He 
left his wife in the small village to give birth to his son and started painting 
at the Nudist Colony. The son, Reuben, is born and, after three months, the 
wife takes him to the Colony in order that her husband might see their off- 
spring; but instead of being happy to see them, he tells her that he has fallen 
in love with a girl at the Colony and points to his painting to show that 
this new love gives him inspiration that he needs for his work. The wife 
is deeply hurt but takes the child back home, hoping that he'll return someday. 

Or he may achieve symbolic reconciliation in death. 

Eugene was deeply in love with Ruth and they were to be married in June. 
All plans were made but on May 20, Eugene received a call to come 
immediately to Ruth's home. She's had a heart attack; but, before Eugene 
could get there, Ruth had died. He worshipped the girl and almost went 
insane. Each night he would dream of walking through the cemetery and 
lingering for hours over her grave. He finally took poison so that he might 
die and be nearer Ruth. 

The disruption of the love relationship may result in further disruption 
of the individual's life. In the extreme case it may even result in insanity or 
death. 

After Louise's death, he was a different man. He lost all his glamour, 
ambition, and his ability to converse with immediate friends. At night, 
instead of going out to make a friendly call on some of his friends, he would 
stay in complete darkness and stare out of the window, his body only 
illuminated by the moon. In the distance he saw nothing, but kept gazing 
at the sky for hours in hopes of seeing Louise. After several weeks of the 
same performance, he went literally insane, and was put away. 

The old man is looking at his wife's grave. He is very dejected. He doesn't 
know what to do with himself. He's thinking over all the things they did 
together before she died. He goes to visit her grave pretty often. He's sort 
of hoping he'll die himself before long he has nothing to do on earth. He 
does a lot of praying. He dies himself before pretty long. 



LOVE, SEX, AND MARITAL RELATIONSHIPS 165 

The individual in such cases is telling us of the importance of his love 
need and how unyielding his fixation to a love object might be. This type 
of story has been told by individuals who have never permitted themselves 
to enter into a love relationship because of their expectation that it would be 
disrupted and their lives shattered by the experience. 

Another reaction to disruption of a love relationship is the quest of a 
new love object. This is achieved with varying degrees of effectiveness and 
residual conflict. In the two stories which follow the development of another 
relationship in one case and the hope of one in the other enable the individual 
to cope with the disruption of the original relationship. 

When she went out to garden this morning she had been perfectly happy. 
She had had everything a home, family, fiance, pleasant work to do and 
wonderful things to look forward to. But she hadn't been out there more than 
an hour or so when she had seen her lover walking along the wood path 
with another girl. From that moment on, it seemed as though the bottom 
had dropped out of her life, present and future, the most important thing and 
her fiance being gone as far as she was concerned. And walking with his 
childhood sweetheart! She had known he still held a small place for her in 
his heart, but had hidden from herself the possibility of its outgrowing her 
own place there. All this she thought as she was sitting down to rest before 
lunch. What could she do now? Well, she finally realized that the only 
thing to do was to say nothing and let him tell her when he wanted to. 
Which is just what he did, and soon after married the girl who "had always 
had a small place in his heart" But time went on and she grew older and 
wiser and realized she had missed nothing by losing him. As if in reward 
for these thoughts, she by and by met a man who really appreciated her and 
understood her. And this man gave his whole heart to her. 

A few years ago, in a rural town, there lived a handsome young man, a 
young girl, and an older woman who was determined to have the young man 
for her husband. The two younger people were really in love, but by means 
of deceit and treachery, the older woman was able to make the man forget 
about the other girl and marry her. Now they live on a prosperous farm 
where he plows and keeps his family protected. The young girl has to pass 
the farm on the way to school, and the wife pretends not to notice her, for 
she knows how much she would like to be mistress of that farm herself. The 
farming family will continue to live together, but without any love or 
companionship. The young girl will get her fill of "book learning" and be 
a career girl while wailing for another "true love** to come along. 

In other cases, the individual, incapable of seeking a new love object or 
unwilling to do so, turns to some other region to cope with the trauma 
suffered in the disruption of the love relationship. In the following story, 
the heroine turns to work to forget. 

This woman is pleading with this man to leave his work for a while and 
relax. She offers herself to him. The man here seems to be hesitating between 



l66 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

his duty and his love, but actually he knows which really matters to him. 
Besides, he will soon grow tired of the woman and the temporary pleasure 
is nothing compared with the rewards of his work. So he fools around with 
her for a while and then throws her off without a remorse because after all 
she got what she wanted and he gave as much as was his to give. The 
woman takes it badly at first but she recovers and throws herself into her own 
work with greater determination. She is thrilled to find how vtfell she makes 
out on her job. 

But the activity may be something other than an attempt to forget. The 
individual may wish to preserve the memory of the love object and prevent 
similar tragedies in the lives of others. 

John Smith was an average American man. He worked in a defense plant 
and earned a pretty good living and felt on top of the world. He felt that 
everything was going OK for him. He was married to this woman. She 
complained of severe pains in the chest. She went to a doctor and was told 
that she was developing tuberculosis. She told John nothing of this for she 
did not want to worry him. He was doing very well in his job and had 
been promoted to floor manager. She continued suffering in silence. Her 
illness required nothing but a few months' rest at a sanatorium with the 
proper climate, food, and attention. One night, John came home from work 
and found his wife lying in bed gasping for breath. He immediately called 
the doctor, who pronounced her case as advanced tuberculosis. She was sent 
away to the sanatorium. But she did not recover and, instead, died. After 
the first shock of her death, he continued working and became head of the 
defense plant, which was now converted back to civilian manufacturing. 
John Smith became a wealthy man and gave huge sums of money to the 
tuberculosis fund so that other women, children, men, and boys, would not 
have to suffer from this dread disease. 

Activity which has its origin in the attempted suppression of painful 
memories may be the basis for a regeneration of interest, which had been 
blunted by grief. 

The girl on the bridge lives down on the waterfront. She often goes down 
in the morning to watch the longshoremen loading and unloading the boats, 
ships I should say. Usually her surroundings gave her a feeling of content- 
ment. They are all that she had ever known and they are all she wants to 
know. This particular morning however, her surroundings bring back only 
painful memories. Painful in the sense that she will never experience them 
again. Her fiancee had worked as a longshoreman on these very docks. And 
they'd been very happy planning their future together. When the war came, 
he joined the Navy and yesterday they had received notice of his death. As 
she looked about her, and down into the murky waters of the river below, she 
wondered whether it was worth while to go on living without him. Lacking 
the nerve to heave herself off the bridge into the river, and as she was a 
practical person, realizing that the men were near enough to fish her out if 
she did, she decided the best way to sever connections with these painful 



LOVE, SEX, AN 7 D MARITAL RELATIONSHIPS l6j 

surroundings was to leave the city and try to make a life for herself elsewhere. 
That afternoon she packed her bags and got on the train, her destination 
being another city quite some distance from her home. She found a place to 
live and got a job in a war factory. Soon she made friends among the girls 
working in her department. Soon she began to enjoy the .interests and plans 
of her friends. She, uh, went to a dance at the U.S.O. and made friends there. 
With all these new contacts, she soon forgot about the grief, she thought was 
going to color the rest of her life. 

Although this may happen, for many individuals substitute satisfactions 
never altogether replace the lost love relationship. 

This ex-service man comes back to meet the woman with whom he thought 
he was in full accord before he left to find that she's not the same girl he dreamt 
about in the horrible days of war. At the point of this picture, some 
fundamental misunderstanding has come up. The man is determined that 
he cannot overlook this and the woman is pleading with him to do so. He 
leaves her because he's a man of determination and strong will, but finds out 
later that in his strength in his trying to be strong 'he had lost the woman 
he'd loved. But he resigns himself to this fact, and leads a life of superficial 
happiness. 

The disruption of the love relation may produce misery and resignation, 
the quest of a new love object or substitute satisfaction. But, frequently, the 
most conspicuous sequelae involve aggression directed either at the love 
object, or the self, or both. 

The story which follows is representative of the purely extrapunitive 
reaction. 

A husband no, change it to a lover, I'll make it real good while I'm at it 
in his jealous rage has killed his beloved. Through his grief one thought still 
shines escape. Tripping down the stairs, bumping into the little old lady on 
the first floor, he runs blindly to the street Escape running through his mind, 
the shock of her death has turned him into an unbalanced person. Running 
down the street at a terrific pace, not knowing where to go, he is led on by 
his conscience pounding "escape" in his brain. Stumbling along, he goes to a 
movie so he will not be caught. Planning every motion carefully in Ms 
twisted mind, he sets out after dinner to the hall where she would have given 
her concert tonight. As far as he could see he had not made a slip. On 
entering the hall he heard thunderous applause. Wondering who it could be 
for, he went into the auditorium where he saw her, bandage on her head but 
otherwise all right. Muttering to himself, "I killed her, I killed her," he 
leaped forward, took her white neck in his hands and slowly drained the life 
from her. Like a madman he leaped up realizing what he had done, but it 
was too late. The wheels of justice had already begun to turn and, as they 
took him away, he was still muttering, *1 killed her, I killed her." 

Aggression incited by disruption of the relationship is not always so 
directly expressed. Thus the hero may be the instrument of the death of his 
love object, even though no overt aggressive act is committed. 



l68 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

This is the ghost of a lover come to keep his tryst by the street lamp. Both 
he and his girl have long been dead but still he comes. She used to be a 
passionate girl and, for a long time, he resisted her. However, when he 
finally veered around, her feelings for him had cooled somewhat. In fact, 
she had found a new interest. He did not realize this. One day, he asked 
her to meet him at a certain place. She never turned up. He waited and 
waited, refusing to believe that she would not come. It was a 'bitterly cold 
night and, as the hours wore on, a numbness overcame him. The next 
morning, they found him lying in a heap by the lampost more dead than 
alive. He died of pneumonia in the hospital. When the girl heard of this 
she was, of course, sorry in a way, but she said what was she to do? It was 
silly of him to wait so long. Besides when she had wanted him to love her, 
he wouldn't. If he had returned her love then, things might have been 
very different 

Or the hero may be the instrument of the accidental death of the love 
object. 

Late one night, this man came home and found that the door to his house 
was locked. He had no key. So he thought that the best plan would be to 
get the stepladder out of the garage. He put the ladder up to a bedroom 
window where the light was on because he thought it would be easier to get 
inside if he could see what he was doing, and it was his and his wife's room. 
He realized that, because the light was still on, his wife must still be up. 
When he climbed to the top of the ladder, he saw his wife standing in the 
middle of the floor facing him. She couldn't see him because the room was 
so light and it was so dark out in the night. She must have mistaken her 
husband for a burglar, because in her hand she held a gun. Before the man 
could think she pulled the trigger. When she discovered her mistake she was 
glad glad that she had killed him. He had been out with that other woman 
for the last time. 

Or death of the love object may be wished, although the hero will not 
commit the aggressive act. 

Well, I think he's going to war. She says she wants him to go. 4< You are 
an American," she says, "and I want you to fight for your country and die 
for It," she says. She's bidding him farewell. "I must tell her before I go," 
he says, "but I don't know how." There's someone else in my life. "She's 
in love with me but I'm not in love with her." You see, she's in love with 
him but he's not in love with her. "I don't believe in marriage without 
love,** he says. "She's in love with me but I'm not in love with her and I 
must tell her there's someone else." And she's saying, "I want you to die a 
noble and honorable death in the service of your country -because I know you 
don't love me." She wants nobody else to have him. 

It is of interest that individuals who are capable in phantasy of killing 
*heir love objects often feel no guilt for their aggression. 

Consider now the hero who turns his aggression against himself. 



LOVE, SEX, AND MARITAL RELATIONSHIPS l6p 

This girl is the girl about whom the "Bridge of Sighs" was written. Now 
before she jumps, she is thinking how bitter has been her life, how pointless, 
how futile, She curses the man she had trusted and jumps. 

In this story, there is hostility towards the once trusted love object, but 
we are uncertain of the extent to which her suicide stems from her bitterness, 
or, the futility of her life. The behavioral vector in all suicide stories would 
be scored vector "against" with the self as subject and object, but the 
Ficudian concept of free-floating aggression which may be turned outward 
or inward is debatable. Rarely do we find in the "intrapunitive" response 
to frustrated love any evidence for an aggressive wish toward the love object 
simply redirected against the self. Aggression against the self, as we shall 
see in some of the stories that follow, may be an act in the service of a 
variety of motives. 

Thus, in the next story, suicide is the means of escaping depression and 
remorse and a social situation which has become unbearable. 

This is the most peculiar cemetery that I have ever seen (long pause laughs 
long pause). This story took place many years ago* This man had married 
a nice girl Throughout his life, he was a respected person in the community, 
but, unknown to mo*t people, he was extremely unkind to his wife, finding 
many subtle ways of making her unhappy. He frequently limited her social 
activities and refused to give her the things she most enjoyed. She died in 
middle life and, as the years went by, her husband gradually became remorseful. 
He became depressed and seclusive. People came to look upon him as a town 
character. Finally, he could stand it no longer. So on a very dark night he 
went to her grave and committed suicide. 

Suicide, in the following story, is the result of a realization that "he had 
lost the only person who really liked him or could stand him," and he had 
become a social outcast. 

Paul Avon and his wife had been very much in love and were very devoted. 
But he was always rather conceited and hot-headed and wanted everything 
the way he liked it. She was very devoted to him but was very popular and 
social. She would have given up anything for him, but his constant nagging, 
jealousy and sureness of himself killed in her all^she loved in him, and, in 
mental anguish, finally left him and remarried later. Soon after her marriage, 
he realized he had lost the only person who really liked him or could stand 
him. And in the world he became an outcast because of his eccentricities, 
and rather than bother her any more, he killed himself in a gas-filled room 
under an assumed name. 

Another example of suicide as a means of relieving a situation which has 
become intolerable is the following story. 

This man has lost his wife to his best friend. He is so terribly in love with 
his wife that he looks only for her happiness and gives her to his best friend. 



lyO THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

After their marriage takes place, they are killed by an automoDile, which 
mangles them to death. The man becomes so despondent that he is sent 
away to a hospital for the insane. The environment plus his sorrows leads 
him to committing suicide, which he has just done. He slumps down on 
the floor, happy that he is out of his misery. 

We have evidence for truly aggressive wishes directed against the self, 
but these rarely involve wishes for physical self destruction. The individual 
may criticize himself for his inadequacy in the love relationship, or feel 
remorse for hurting the love object, but, if he takes his life, he does this to 
escape his inner misery, which may be caused by loneliness, his sense of 
inadequacy, social ostracism, or his feelings of guilt, which are unbearable. 
There is rarely, however, a hero who "wishes" suicide for its own sake. For 
this reason, the extreme intrapunitive response must be regarded as typically 
a means to some other end rather than an end in itself. He may "hate" him- 
self as he once hated his love object, but killing the love object is an expres- 
sion of the hatred, whereas self-destruction is very rarely a direct expression 
of self hatred. 



II. DIMENSIONS OF THE SEX REGION 

Although love and marital adjustment are closely related to sexual 
adjustment, there is, in our society, sufficient discrepancy in the expression of 
these related needs to justify a separate scrutiny of the dimensions of these 
two regions. Precisely the same dimensions used in the analysis of love can 
be employed in the analysis of the sex need. 

A. THE NATURE OF THE SEX NEED 

I. THE TYPE OF SEX NEED 

a) Varieties of Heterosexual Relationships 

Inspection of TAT stories reveals that every type of love relationship has 
its sexual counterpart, but that for any individual the love relationship may 
be defined quite differently from the sexual relationship. 

In the following story, the sexual experience is exciting because of its 
fusion with the wish to be cared for (vector "on"). 

lanie nas just come in from a thrilling date with Bob. She started to get 
ready for bed. She is so excited she's walking on air. She sits dreaming 
about it, treasuring over every moment He's so wonderful, the most exciting 
man she's ever known. When he kisses her, tiny shivers run up and down 
Her spine and she just melts. He's so tall and handsome and makes her feel 
so warm and protected. A girl likes to have a man she can lean on. 



LOVE, SEX, AND MARITAL RELATIONSHIPS 17! 

The complementary wish, to help the love object (vector "for") is tused 
with the sex need in this story. 

When she met him she liked him immediately. He reminded her o a lost 
pup. She immediately took him in hand in her friendly way and they started 
having dinner together after work. Then one evening he suggested that she 
come to his apartment so they could talk. She went and he began to tell 
her how much knowing her had meant to him. She was very touched and 
then the last thing she expected happened. He came over and kissed her 
passionately and told her that he wanted her. She tries to look shocked but 
he's so impulsive and childlike that she finds herself kissing him back. She's 
more excited than she ever thought she could be. Suddenly, she wants to 
give herself to him his need of her is so grea*- 

The wish to govern the love object (vector "over") may be fused with 
the sex need. 

This guy brought this girl home after doing the night spots. He asked her 
if he could come in for a while. She isn't in favor of it, but he finally 
convinces her. She mixes him a nightcap and he starts pawing her. She 
objects, but this only adds fuel to the fire and the idea of making her give 
in makes him more ardent. He's doing fine until her roommate comes in. 

Or the complementary need to be governed (vector "by") may be 
satisfied in the sexual experience. 

This scene shows a seduction which has just taken place. She was a virgin 
and didn't quite want to do it. But he was so masterful and she got so 
excited she couldn't help giving in. 

Sex may also be the vehicle of shared intimacy (vector "with"). 

A woman who has slept with her lover for the first time, sitting and dreaming 
about it. It was a wonderful experience. She never felt so close to anyone in 
her life. She knows now what they mean when they say united as one. 

Or it may be based on esthetic enjoyment of the love object (vector 
"toward"). 

All during the war, in the foxholes and on the battlefield, he could still sec 
her as she had been that night, lying in bed, half naked, her beautifully curved 
body relaxed, her lips slightly parted. He grew hot and cold by turns as he 
thought of the way she had looked. 

A variation of the same wish is expressed in voyeurism. 

This is a picture of a Peeping Tom who's been causing a great commotion 
in the neighborhood by looking in windows and watching the women get 
undressed. The men of the neighborhood have organized a group to look 
for him. He hears them coming and slides down the rope to try to get 
away. He gets away this time, but they finally catch him. 



j^2 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

Still another variant is the exhibitionistic wish to evoke interest in the 
self as a sex object, (wish-other object-vector "toward" object-self). 

The better he got to know them, the more he desired them but he desired 
both of them equally and he had an idea that some day he'd go over and 
pull his penis out in front of them and see how they both reacted. So he 
did But they weren't even shocked and still he couldn't choose between them. 

The sexual experience may have as its primary meaning the acquisition 
and possession of the love object (vector "from"). 

This man is standing at the open window thinking about the evening he 
just spent. He's gone with this girl for a long time but he's never really 
possessed her, sexually, that is. He's tried but she always seems so unapproach- 
able. He wants this very much and he's dreaming of the day when she will 
be his completely. 

There is also a type of sexual satisfaction fused with the wish to hurt 
(vector ""against"). 

This guy's a "queer", some sort of a sex pervert. He gets a big kick out of 
beating women and hearing them scream, and then, when they're just about 
passing out, he lays them. 

Related is sexual enjoyment in being hurt. This may be expressed as 
wish-other object-vector "against" object-selt. 

They've been fighting, having an awful row about something. All of a sudden 
he loses his temper completely and slaps her. Instead of making her mad, it 
excites her, she doesn't understand it. It has a strange effect on her. In the 
future she finds herself deliberately provoking him and aggravating him to 
the point where he'll slap her again. 

b) Varieties of Homosexual Relationships 

We must also examine the stories for evidence of homosexual wishes. 
Stories are sometimes told by men in which the storyteller identifies with a 
woman in a sexual or love relationship. We see this in the following story. 

She's in love with him but she suspects that he's been seeing other women. 
She finally finds proof of this something in his bureau drawer and she's 
completely broken up. She was all right until she knew it for sure, but now 
she has to face the fact and she can't. She alternates between crying and 
cursing him. She commits suicide rather than have to live with this knowledge. 

Identification with women, based on identification with the mother, is 
not infrequent in American society and is reflected in the TAT's of many 
heterosexual males. Whether or not this results in an "effeminate" male is 
to some extent determined by the personality of the mother with whom the 



LOVE, SEX, AND MARITAL RELATIONSHIPS 173 

son identifies. In many cases, the mother's personality coincides with the 
cultural definition of the masculine character. Conversely, there are sons 
who, in identifying with an "effeminate" father, are less masculine than those 
identifying with masculine mothers. For this reason we cannot assume that 
female identification in the stories is related in any simple manner to homo- 
sexuality. 

We must further distinguish an unconscious identification with, the 
opposite sex from a wish to be of the other sex. The male may know himself 
to be a man and wish to be a woman, or think himself effeminate and wish 
he could be a "real man." In the following story, told by a girl who had 
been a tomboy in her youth and relinquished the masculine role with regret, 
we see another aspect of this problem the person who wishes to be of 
another sex and also loves objects of that sex. 

She's sitting in a room looking out of a window and she's watching her 
brothers and their friends outside playing baseball. It's spring. She's very 
proud of her brothers but she's also wishing she were a boy and could play 
with them too. She's about fifteen years old. What happens to her is she 
reverts from her tomboy wishes and aspirations and becomes extremely 
feminine in a couple of years. She sort of realizes she can't be a boy, uncon- 
sciously, and she decides that, if she can't, at least she wants them to notice her 
some way or another, and she does it by becoming very popular so that all her 
brothers* friends take her out. 

In this case, the heroine did not identify with boys, since she knew she 
was not and could not be one, but she did wish to be a boy. But not so 
that she might indulge in lesbian activities, since her preferred love objects 
were also male. There are also males who wish to be females and who have 
females as love objects. 

The following story illustrates the case in which the individual feels 
identified with the opposite sex, but wishes he were a better representative of 
his own sex. 

This boy was raised by two maiden aunts who insisted that he wear Little Lord 
Fauntlcroy suits and learn to play the violin. He doesn't mind this when he's 
small, but when he grows up and goes to college, especially when he starts 
going out with girls, he feels that there is a difference between him and the 
other fellows and the girls seem to feel it, too. He wishes there were some- 
thing he could do about it but he doesn't quite know what 

As we have seen, the choice of homosexual or heterosexual object is 
independent of the sex with which the individual feels identified or the sex 
he wishes he were. Either heterosexual or homosexual objects may be 
sought by men who are either identified with men or wctnen or who may 
wish to be either masculine or feminine. 



174 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

In the following story, we see the homosexual object choice on the part 
of the effeminate man. 

A weak, effeminate, most repulsive-looking character. A boy who grew up in 
a world of his own. No one ever understood him. He may have been a 
musician, dramatist might have been an actor became weak and fleshy. 
He's enjoying being in the clutches of this man. He gives himself up freely 
(I wanted to find someone who would rule my life) . He has a strong sensuous 
feeling about it. He is on his way out now. He will end up by suicide. Or, 
having lost his self respect completely, he will sink down into the depths of 
degredation. 

We next see the same type of object-choice on the part of a man who 
wishes to be a woman only because he wishes to enjoy homosexual objects, 
but his wish is not to be a woman per se. 

This guy is a prize fighter * real rugged guy. He's never gone out with 
women very much. He always liked men better. He's a little peculiar and 
sometimes he dreams of being a woman so he could have a man. 

The homosexual object-choice may also stem from a man's wish to be 
a woman, per se. 

Is that a man or a woman? I think he's a man but a queer. He wants to 
be a woman and he dresses up in a woman's clothes and imagines himself 
feeling like a woman and flirting with men. 

Another variety of homosexual object-choice is based on masculine identi- 
fication and masculine object-choice. The man has no interest whatsoever in 
women and enjoys homosexual experience for its own sake. This type of 
story is extremely rare in the protocols of heterosexual males but not 
infrequent in the stories of overt homosexuals. 

A picture of a sailor climbing a rope. He's been going to sea ever since he 
was a kid. He's a homosexual and part of the attraction of the life at sea 
is that all the other men are too. He was introduced to this by the first mate 
and continued it ever since then. 

Or the individual may have purely homosexual wishes which he cannot 
accept and which he feels are degrading. 

Hm, interesting picture, a young boy who (I'm getting a sexual reaction from 
this picture) he was put to sleep by the man. The boy has tendencies toward 
weakness. He either lived with the man or was drawn to the man. He 
couldn't help himself. The old man has hypnotic power over him and is 
putting him in a deep trance. The man is very bitter with life and he found 
a young man clean of mind and body. The boy comes back to the man. The 
old man has degrading thoughts. The old man will drag him down. . . . 



LOVE, SEX, AND MARITAL RELATIONSHIPS 175 

In some cases, previous homosexual experience may interfere with the 
development of heterosexual interests. 

Young man in youth had had homosexual relations, and these always made 
him fear to have sex relations with women, which he really wanted. On one 
occasion, such a relationship did develop and he became tormented because the 
idea of homosexual relations were always on his mind; he couldn't enjoy her 
it made him very unhappy. After this relationship, he went out and committed 
suicide. 

That the interrelationship between homosexual and heterosexual wishes 
may be complex is illustrated in a story told by an individual whose primary 
conflict centered on his feelings of inadequacy as a male. 

Hard to determine sex and what's on the floor. I can think of a story but it's 
so disgusting Fm afraid. This person is a boy, but he's always wanted to be 
a girl. One day, he's so sick of fighting it off, or trying to make believe he's 
like other boys, that he dresses up as a girl. He stuffs artificial breasts into his 
dress, etc. He decides he's going to fuck himself. So he looks around for 
something that'll look like a penis. He hasn't decided where he's going to 
put it. So he finds this old splinter of wood that's on the floor and he wraps 
a cloth around it so it won't hurt He sticks the thing in between the cushion 
and the frame and it's sticking out like an erect penis. And then he gets all 
ready and he himself is sexually excited and he becomes frustrated because he 
realizes that there's no place in his anatomy to fit this thing So he 
masturbates, and while he's masturbating, he takes the role of the male and has 
a heterosexual revery while he's masturbating. Then, when he's all done, 
he feels disgusted just because he's masturbated and because he's dressed up 
like a girl and he feels he shouldn't be* So he sits down and he cries. And 
while he cries, he feels how hopeless it is; he feels he needs a male in his 
arms and although it is not sexual, he feels he needs a male to protect him. 
And he doesn't know what to do. 

Of interest in this story is the reference to masturbation. Although the 
practice of masturbation is admittedly common, this is one of the few 
protocols, in our experience, in which there is direct reference to the practice. 
This is puzzling inasmuch as other equally tabooed wishes* and practices are 
mentioned far more frequently by normal subjects. We have not, however, 
been able to find an explanation for this. 

2. RECIPROCITY IN THE SEXUAL RELATIONSHIP 

Let us consider the degree of reciprocity in the sexual relationship. Is 
the ardor of the hero returned by his sexual partner or is the hero more or 
less responsive than she? Sex in the following story is completely unrecip- 
rocal; the hero rapes the girl, who "is unaware of the action that has taken 
place." 



Ij6 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

This is an obvious case of rape from the picture. The girl is motionless, 
indicating that she is unaware of the action that has taken place. The man, 
from the pained expression on his face and the fact that he has his arm in 
front of his eyes, is terribly ashamed and full of remorse. He sits beside her 
until she comes to in an attempt to square himself with her. He appears to 
have some strength of character in his face despite his cowardly act. 

In marked contrast, sex is completely reciprocated in the next story. 

Ahhh . . . He's just sort of weary . . . Well, let's see now. Can't figure why 
he's got his clothes on. Well, he got a date with this gal and they've been 
necking; they're always necking. But tonight, it's petting pretty heavy . . . 
pretty heavy petting. Oh yah, and they're going further and further and 
then suddenly someone's turned the light and he jumps up fast. And he's 
got his hand over his face. He's feeling faint. You know how you feel 
faint when you get up fast And the light's in his eyes. It's a very traumatic 
moment She looks the other way and she'll pull the covers over her head. Her 
roommate in the apartment house. This is her room but she lives with a girl 
friend who's as shocked as they are. But she didn't know the girl would be 
in this position she is. And so she goes out quietly and leaves the light on. 
They're very disturbed. But they leave the light on and talk awhile about 
public relations and sex and finally they convince themselves that the public be 
damned. He undresses and snaps the light off and they have a fine time. 
They're even happier now, because now they've talked about it. They hadn't 
verbalized it before. And talking all these things over made it all right. 

Between these two points we find the same range of variability that we 
saw in the reciprocity of the love relationship. In the following story, for 
example, there is neither complete reciprocity nor a complete lack of 
reciprocity in the sexual relationship. 

This poor guy wants to go to bed with this girl, but he's so afraid of being 
impotent that half the time he ends up that way just because he's afraid. She's 
luring him on and gets into bed with her breast exposed. He's excited but 
he*s so afraid of making a fool of himself that he walks out and leaves her 
wondering what happened. 

3. INTENSITY OF THE SEX NEED 

Direct references to the intensity of the sex need are relatively infrequent; 
more common are references to the degree of enjoyment or excitement in 
sexual experience. Although this may be a resultant of the basic sex need, 
modified by inhibiting forces, it yet provides an index of the intensity of the 
need as it operates within the total personality. The three stories which, 
follow are a sample of the range of variability of enjoyment and excitement. 

This guy has had his first experience with sex, and, if he were to be perfectly 
frank about it, he would admit that it was a little disappointing. He enjoyed 
it, but he wonders what the hell everyone has been raving about for years. 



LOVE, SEX, AND MARITAL RELATIONSHIPS 177 

These two people met at a dance hall and end up in bed together. Afterwards, 
he leaves. He enjoyed himself a lot because he hadn't been with a woman for 
a long time. He goes home feeling very good. 

It's hard to think of these people as real. They look so unreal and so 
fictitious. She's a famous singer in a night club in New York. He has met 
her; he's a janitor; he comes to clean up. He's very dumb handsome but 
dumb type. He's met her after hours while he's cleaning up around the place. 
He feels that she'd be a good lay and vica versa to put it bluntly. The trouble 
is, he feels he's so inferior to her because he realizes he has no talent and is 
kind of dumb. But she's asked him up to her apartment. (This is habitual 
and, every time he's gone up, he's laid her.) Well, she's very kind and she 
understands how he feels and she's always trying to give him confidence 'in 
himself; but the more gentle she is with him, the more he resents it and 
feels she's patronizing him. One afternoon he goes up to the apartment (she 
asked him up) and she tells him she wants him to learn to play the drums so he 
can play in the orchestra in the night club where she sings. He resents this be- 
cause he feels that she just wants to keep him around because she likes his body 
and there's nothing else about him which she values. And so instead of just 
laying her today, he suddenly becomes very vicious very aggressive, I should 
say the word is and he gets her on the bed and rips off her clothes and starts 
to bite her breasts. She is it hurts her and she's sort of struggling, but 
she's tremendously excited sexually so he starts to slap her hard and she 
starts to cry and he feels at this point that he's triumphed and then after this 
triumph he becomes very gentle, very tender with her and gives her the best 
screwing she's ever had. (I like this story makes me feel kinda good.) He 
agrees that he'll learn how to play the drums and he works terrifically hard at 
it arid he makes a success and they get married and live happily ever after. 

4. TEMPORAL CHARACTERISTICS OF THE SEX NEED 

The temporal characteristics of the sexual relationship may be analyzed 
in the same way as those of the love relationship, 

a) Maturation 

Does the hero typically engage in sexual activity or have sexual wishes 
on first meeting or only after a longer acquaintance? In the three stories 
which follow we see a sample of variability in maturation. 

These two people met at a dance hall and end up in bed together. After- 
wards, he leaves. He enjoyed himself a lot because he hadn't been with a 
woman for a long time. He goes home feeling very good. 

When she met him she liked him immediately. He reminded her of a lost 
pup. She immediately took him in hand in her friendly way and they started 
having dinner together after work. Then one evening he suggested that she 
come to his apartment so they could talk. She went and he began to tell her 
how much knowing her bad meant to him. She was very touched and then 
the last thing she expected happened. He came over and kissed her passionately 



178 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

and told her that he wanted her. She tries to look shocked but he's so 
impulsive and childlike that she finds herself kissing him back. She's more 
excited than she ever thought she could be. Suddenly she wants to give herself 
to him his need of her is so great. 

These people have known each other for years. They both work in the same 
place and were always interested in each other. One night they're leaving 
the office, both having been working late at night. He waits for her to turn 
out the lights and as she comes through the hall they're very close together 
and there's a sort of animal magnetism in the air. The next thing they know, 
they're in each other's arms. 

b) Duration 

Duration of the sexual relationship is a dimension which may vary 
independently of its maturation. 

This is a girl this fellow has picked up. Here we see him full of remorse. He 
vows he will never see her again. 

Shown here are a man and his mistress. They have come to a parting of the 
ways. They have had a lot of fun together but lately they are getting on each 
other's nerves, and this is it 

He's married but he's been having an affair with this woman. They can't get 
married because of his wife and children. So I guess they'll just go on 
like this. 

B. THE VICISSITUDES OF THE SEXUAL RELATIONSHIP 

It is a commentary on the relative importance of the love and sex need 
in American culture that although the heroes of TAT stories are concerned 
with the duration of the love relationship, it is a very exceptional hero who is 
anxious about the duration of a strictly sexual relationship. Though the 
hero's life may be shattered by the disruption of a love relationship, there 
are no such sequelae to the disruption of a sex relationship nor are the 
poignant attempts to reconstitute a disrupted love relationship ever paralleled 
in the quest of a lost sex object. But if the sex need is taken less seriously 
in this respect, it is nonetheless beset with anxieties peculiar to the particular 
pressures of American culture. 

I. DEGREE OF INHIBITION OF SEXUAL BEHAVIOR 

The most extreme degree of inhibition of the sex need is exemplified by 
those individuals who are incapable of experiencing sexual feeling of any 
kind or of entertaining any sexual phantasies. We sometimes find this 
extreme inhibition attributed to the hero of a story. 

This woman never married. She's an old maid. She's not like most old 
maids. She was never interested in the things that interested other women, 



LOVE, SEX, AND MARITAL RELATIONSHIPS 179 

and she never wanted to get married. She spent her life doing good works 
among the poor and she's perfectly content. 

A less severe degree of inhibition allows the individual awareness of his 
wishes but at the cost of severe endopsychic conflict. 

This man is being torn asunder by the struggle between his natural desires 
and the voice of his conscience. He finally masters his feelings and his 
conscience gives him peace. 

Somewhat less inhibited is the individual who can accept his own 
sexuality but who fears the rejection of his wishes by other individuals. 

He's thinking of the girl he met tonight He's thinking of how he would 
like to take her in his arms, but he's afraid that she would be shocked by the 
depth of his feelings. Finally, he goes to sleep. 

Fear of the consequences, or anticipatory guilt, may completely inhibit 
the expression of the sex need, as in the following stories. 

He's wondering if he should sleep with her. Maybe he is wondering if he 
will get VIX if this is someone he has picked up, or maybe he is wondering 
if he will be impotent. He decides it isn't worth taking the chance. 

The girl is tight and the man has taken her home. He knows that he could 
seduce her tonight, but he knows he'll feel ashamed if he does. So he leaves. 

Or sexual behavior may be permitted some expression but never con- 
summated because of guilt, fear of the consequences, or feelings of 
inadequacy. 

This young impulsive young man has been violently in love with this woman. 
He has known her but little and has at no time been with her in any intimate 
situations. Through a coincidence, he and the girl manage to end up in his 
small apartment. He makes an attempt to become familiar with her in a 
physical sense and she refutes him. He offers her a soft drink which he 
dopes, then undresses her, puts her in his cot, and covers her over. At this 
point of this picture, he has just picked up the cover to see her bare breasts 
and is instantly struck with the deep sense of having done wrong. He dresses 
her, revives her, tells her that she has fainted, and they part friends, the 
woman never knowing what has happened and he all the more, capable of 
controlling his emotions for the present. 

The couple in the picture have sneaked up to her room in the boarding house 
to have some fun. They're both a little tight and quite excited. But^ as he 
starts to undress her, they both start to think of what might happen 
supposing she gets pregnant, supposing someone finds out about it This has 
a sobering effect on both of them and it doesn't seem like as much fun as it 
did they feel sort of let down. So he kisses her goodnight and leaves. 

This poor guy wants to go to bed with this girl, but he's so afraid of being 
impotent that half the time he ends up that way just because he's afraid. She's 



I#0 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

luring him on and gets into bed with her breast exposed. He's excited but he*s 
so afraid of making a fool of himself that he walks out and leaves her 
wondering what happened. 

Or the sexual act may be consummated, with remorse or fear of 
consequences following the act. 

The girl has just previously had (illicit) sexual intercourse for the first time 
in her life. Now she is very unhappily thinking about the consequences. The 
chances of having a baby, of getting a bad reputation, etc., all run through 
her mind. This experience will greatly affect this girl, who, because of the 
surroundings, seems to be poor. She will in the future try to do nothing amiss 
and aim her relationship with males towards marriage. 

This fellow has just had intercourse with this woman. It is the first time 
for both of them. She's asleep and he's disgusted with himself because he 
realizes that he doesn't love her and that she will be hurt. He leaves her a 
note saying that it will be better if he never sees her again, that he leave before 
he does any more harm. 

This is an obvious case of rape from the picture. The girl is motionless, 
indicating that she is unaware of the action that has taken place. The man 
from the pained expression on his face, and the fact that he has his arm in 
front of his eyes is terribly ashamed and full of remorse. He sits beside her 
until she comes to in an attempt to square himself with her. He appears to 
have some strength of character in his face despite his cowardly act. 

Strong inhibition of the sexual need may appear in the stories in a more 
oblique way. Thus in stories told by individuals who suffer severe anxiety or 
guilt concerning their sexual wishes, the hero may be allowed to consummate 
the sexual act, but the consequences are extreme. The individual either takes 
his life, is imprisoned, or killed. 

They had been posing for a portrait. That was all right, in and of itself. But, 
they were also very madly and emotionally in love. The artist had gone out 
of the room for a few moments, and in that time the two models had been 
behaving outrageously. They were making love in a barbaric way. Just then 
the artist's wife appeared in the room carrying her baby. She was furious and 
disgusted. The female model was shy and afraid, but. the other model wasn't 
the least bit worried. The wife asked them to dress immediately and leave. 
Her husband was far ahead enough in his work so that he didn't need them 
any longer. But the wife didn't leave it at that. She spread the story about 
and made it appear as filthy and disgusting as possible. There was really no 
need for that. The young couple were not accepted anywhere, either as 
friends or as models. Finally, they gave up in despair and committed suicide 
together. 

This is a picture of a Peeping Tom who's been causing a great commotion in 
the neighborhood by looking in windows and watching the women get 
undressed. The men of the neighborhood have organized a group to look 



LOVE, SEX, AND MARITAL RELATIONSHIPS 151 

for him. He hears them coming and slides down the rope to try to get 
away. He gets away this time, but they finally catch him. 

Stories of sexual wishes or behavior which end in guilt on the one 
hand or punishment by some external force on the other, are generally repre- 
sentative of the degree of introjection of conscience with respect to sexuality. 
The individual who feels remorse for his sexuality has introjected the 
parental or societal dictate to a greater extent than the individual who sees 
himself being imprisoned for sexuality. This is illustrated in one case* 
analyzed by the writer in which either guilt or punishment by the law 
followed the consummation of the sex act. These sequelae occurred, how- 
ever, in alternation so that when the hero of the story felt guilt there was no 
external punishment and when there was external punishment, no guilt 
feelings were expressed. In the following two stories from this protocol we 
see the alternation of these sequelae. 

The man has succeeded in getting the woman drunk and then having inter- 
course with her. Now, he is getting ready to leave but he cannot quite 
coordinate between drunkenness and sleepiness. He knows he must leave but 
he doesn't know why. He forgets his suit coat^ and out on the street he is 
arrested. 

The man is very drunk. In this condition he has gone to see the woman 
shown here. She has gotten undressed and into bed and is now pretending 
to pay no attention to him and to be disinterested. He is getting undressed 
in order to get into bed with her. In the moment shown, he has gained some 
of his faculties and is at this moment thinking how wrong it is to do this 
thing he has contemplated. Immediately, however, liquor will cloud his mind 
and his body will take control. Afterwards, he will probably be very upset 
about it 

In another story from the same protocol the hero himself tells us of 
the existence of this alternation. 

The boy is a collector of old wood and is very much attracted to the block 
holding the library bell up. When the guard is not looking, he whittles off a 
piece and rushes out. Later, he comes back and starts to glue it on but is 
caught and put into jail for 99}^ years. He escaped from jail but then he 
failed to get guilt feelings. So, he got so worried he turned himself in. 

Finally, there are ,stoiies in which the consummation of the sex act pro- 
ceeds without incident. This does not necessarily signify that the individual 
telling the story enjoys the same degree of freedom in sexual behavior. In 
the case just cited, for example, where references both to remorse and to 
incarceration and death for the expression of sexual wishes are frequent, we 
also find the following story in which the hero is unhampered by inhibition, 

* This is the subject who was given daily TATi. 



l82 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

The woman has been lying in the dark, naked. A man she knows is staying 
in the same house. She has had the servant tell him his room was the one 
she is now in. The man has opened the door and the light shines on her 
body. Realizing the charms of her body, she merely turns her head aside as 
if to betoken modesty. He stammers an apology and starts to leave but seeing 
that she doesn't seem to mind his presence he stays to "chat." They spend the 
night together. 

The key to an understanding of this paradox lies in the phrase "she 
doesn't seem to mind" as well as in the woman's general initiative in the 
sexual relationship. Whenever the hero initiates the sexual relationship, 
either the woman minds very much, breaking off the relationship completely, 
or he is overcome with remorse or imprisoned. The story tells us the unique 
condition under which he might be able to express his sexual wishes. Contrast 
the above story with the following, also told by this subject. 

This couple are dancing together. They have been old friends for a long 
time. Now the man tells the woman that he loves her. He kisses her lightly 
on the forehead. In a moment, he'll kiss her passionately on the lips. Then 
she'll tell him that she doesn't love him and not to put her in a position where 
she'll have to forfeit his friendship by not allowing him to see her. 

The man while suddenly kissing the girl on the cheek bit her cheek. He 
apologized profusely he could [slip of the pen] tell why he had done it. 
No avail their acquaintance was broken off. 

Stories of uninhibited consummation of sexuality may be told by 
extremely inhibited individuals, as above, but they may also mean what they 
seem to say: that sex is not beset with crippling anxieties. As in any inter- 
pretation of a single story, this can only be determined by an analysis of the 
entire protocol. 

2, NATURE OF INHIBITION OF SEXUAL BEHAVIOR 

Having considered the range of inhibition of the sexual need, let us turn 
to a closer examination of the nature of these inhibitions. 

a) Exogenous sources 

As we have noted in previous examples, sexual behavior may be inhibited 
by a variety of fears stemming primarily from exogenous sources. 

Common to both men and women is a fear of pregnancy, particularly in 
premarital relations. 

Joe was always a conscientious guy and he worried a lot about the family's 
financial situation, and Paul was, on the contrary, very heedless. He didn't 
have any sense of responsibility. As they grew older, started getting aware of 
life, they sort of broke away from the gang. They started going around 



LOVE, SEX, AND MARITAL RELATIONSHIPS 183 

about 14 or 15 very young with girls who were going around with each 
other and they also started quite early having sexual intercourse with them. 
Joe was always afraid of it and was afraid if she were to have a baby and 
Paul didn't worry much because he wasn't responsible. Joe, when he was 
about 17, he became, against his wishes, a father. So iie married the girL He 
was poor and she was poor and he didn't have money to support the girl and 
he had to help his mother, too. 

Another source of anxiety is the possibility of disease. 

He's wondering if he should sleep with her. Maybe he is wondering if he 
will get V.D. if this is someone he has picked up; or maybe he is wondering 
if he will be impotent. He decides it isn't worth taking the chance. 

Or the individual may fear general social disapprobation if his sexual behavior 
should be discovered. 

This couple have just had intercourse and he's worried about how he's going 
to get out of the building without being seen. He tiptoes all the way out and 
jumps at his own shadow. When he finally gets out, he breathes a sigh of 
relief, but he thinks they shouldn't go on like this supposing he had been 
caught. 

Or more specifically, he may fear arrest or imprisonment for his sexual 
behavior. 

This guy has just committed rape. He raped this girl. She's passed out and 
he knows he's got to get out of there. He's afraid he'll be caught He passes 
a cop on the street and he has a terrific impulse to run. He's afraid the girl 
will report it; so he takes the next train out of town. 

Rejection, however, may be feared only from the sexual object. 

The guy's worrying. He's been out for the evening and come in and he can't 
sleep. So he's standing in front of the open window, worrying. He's worried. 
He was out with a girl he likes a lot He kissed her goodnight and almost 
was more passionate than he meant to be. He's worried about whether she 
noticed it and whether she thought he was too forward. 

b) Endogenous Sources 

The individual's anxiety may, on the other hand, have its primary source 
within the individual". Thus, he may fed generally inadequate and sexually 
impotent. 

This poor guy wants to go to bed with this girl, but he's so afraid of being 
impotent that half the time he ends up that way just because he's afraid. She's 
luring him on and gets into bed with her breast exposed. He's excited, but 
he's so afraid of making a fool of himself that he walks out and leaves Her 
wondering what happened. 



184 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

Or his ideals may conflict with the free expression of his sexual wishes. 

This man is being torn asunder by the struggle between his natural desires 
and the voice of his conscience. He finally masters his feelings and his 
conscience gives him peace. 

Or the inhibition may stem from the social definition of acceptable sexual 
expression which the hero has introjected. 

This girl and boy are both virgins. They're going to be married and they 
decide there's no sense in waiting a few days doesn't make any difference. 
They're in her room in the hotel and she's half undressed and they're both 
pretty much carried away by emotion but all of a sudden he gets up and says 
"No, it shouldn't be this way, everything should be perfect, we'll be married in 
a few days and then you'll be my wife. We can wait until then." 

III. INTERPRETATION OF THE DIMENSIONS OF THE LOVE, 
MARITAL AND SEX REGIONS 

Having sampled some of the dimensions important in the analysis of the 
love, marital, and sex region, we shall now consider the interpretation of 
these dimensions for the light this may throw on the adjustment of the 
individual in the love and sex region and to illustrate techniques of inter- 
pretation which may also be used in the analysis of parent-child and social 
relationships.* 

CASE OF NANCY 

It should again be stressed that there is no simple relationship between 
phantasy productions and the personality of the individual. We cannot on 
inspection of a story, for example, which ends in suicide after an unhappy 
love affair, assume either that the individual has had an unhappy love affair 
or expects to, or that he is going to commit suicide or expects he would if 
he were rejected. As we have seen in the previous discussion of multifactor 
analysis, it may have one of many meanings. Its exact meaning can be 
ascertained only by a comparison of this story with other stories told by the 
same individual and, sometimes, only by further information from other 

* We have omitted this type of analysis in the family and social regions so that we might 
consider in greater detail other types of regional analysis, which are also applicable to this 
region. Thus we might have inquired in the case of love and sex, a*s we did in connection 
with the family, into the general importance of the region in the economy of the individual. 
The same criteria, e.g., introduction of love objects, number of love stories, range of impact 
of the love region on other regions, and conflict with other regions, may be used in the 
assessment of the importance of love and sex for the individual. We may also analyze 
changes in the love relationship as a function of the age of the hero. The impact of other 
regions on love may also be analyzed. We will assume that the application of the general 
methods employed in other regions does not require detailed illustration here. 



LOVE, SEX, AND MARITAL RELATIONSHIPS 185 

sources. In general, we must employ those techniques of analysis outlined 
in Chapter IV, if we are to avoid the pitfalls of our own projective mechanism 
in interpretation. 

Consider the following story told by a young woman, whom we have 
called Nancy. 

It's very late at night and this girl has come down to the beach by herself. TJiis 
other girl in the picture isn't there at all, this is just an image of the other 
girl as she sees herself. She's very depressed and alone and has come to the 
beach because she wants to dream. She's thinking of a time in the past when 
she was down here with a man. They were at a dance and came here and 
had a quarrel and she ran away and he didn't follow her and he just left 
and she never saw him again. It was a couple of years ago. What she does 
is come down to remember him and see herself as she was. I don't know 
what happens to her. She never sees him again but jumps into the water and 
drowns herself. 

How shall we interpret this story? Is this individual on the point of 
suicide? Has she been rejected? Is this a picture of what she would do if 
she were rejected? On the basis of this story any of these interpretations 
are plausible. But let us turn to another story. 

She isn't half as miserable as she looks. She's had a fight with someone in 
the other room behind her, probably a man. She wants him to think that she's 
quite miserable. She's young and she's seen too many movies and she's waiting 
for him to call her back. She really isn't crying underneath that hair. He's 
too smart and he doesn't call her, and she has nothing to do but close the 
door. After that she really is unhappy and so she learns her lesson and 
grows up. 

Here we see the story of another quarrel and another rejection, but there 
is no suicide even though she "really is unhappy." Why then does she commit 
suicide in one case and not in the other? We can clearly discard one 
hypothesis that rejection is the cause of suicide since she is rejected in both 
stories. Nor can we now accept the hypothesis that she is on the point of 
suicide since this is not an invariant outcome. We must then examine these 
two stories for those differences which might provide an explanation. In the 
second story, she solves her problem by this formula: "She learns her lesson 
and grows up." Is there anything in the first story which would make it 
impossible for the heroine* to "grow up?" We are told that she has "come to 
the beach because she wants to dream." This suggests an antithesis between 
introversion, the cathexis of the inner world, and a more realistic "growing 
up*" With this as a hypothesis, let us see if there is any corroborating 
evidence that introversion is dangerous and that it leads to suicide. We find 
this evidence in another story. 



l86 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

This is a girl who has she's like Miniver Cheevy, sort of, she's looking at the 
various paintings, and she wishes she could be living in some other time and 
she's just a schoolgirl and an introvert and she thinks also that the girl in the 
picture she's looking at is someone like her, a dreamer, and also wishing 
she were somewhere else besides the farm. This girl wants to get away from 
school and all the things that everybody else is doing. She wants to be free 
of all conventions. She "looks and looks and goes on reading" instead of 
"drinking." I don't know what will happen to her, I think that she'll 
probably commit suicide like Virginia Woolf. She won't ever be happy. 

This is further evidence for discarding the hypothesis that it is rejection 
which produces suicide since suicide is here otherwise produced, and as we saw 
before, rejection may or may not result in suicide. We now can better under- 
stand why "dreaming" and "introversion" lead to suicide, since this inner life 
is centered on a wish to throw off convention. On the basis of this evidence, 
a further hypothesis is suggested: that the contrast between introversion, dream- 
ing, and growing up is also a contrast between an unconventional and a con- 
ventional life, and that the unconventional life ends in suicide. If this were 
the case, we should expect to find other stories in which people who live 
unconventional lives also commit suicide. We find, on inspection of other 
stories, evidence that women who do unconventional things die, although 
they do not commit suicide. 

This is a very nice fine girl. And she is a secretary to this man. She came 
from Peoria to New York City, very nice and innocent. He asks her to go out 
with him and he takes her up to his apartment. Right here he's asking her 
if she'll have a drink and she's very shocked and then he convinces her it's all 
right to drink, it's the thing to do. She takes a drink and gets drunk and he 
seduces her. Then she tries, the next day thinks whether she should go back 
to Peoria or stay in New York, now that she's a little confused about everything. 
All sorts of awful things are going to happen to her; she's such a dope I She 
has an abortion and she dies and that's the end of her. 

We have evidence, then, that an unconventional life may lead either to 
suicide or death. Suicide is preceded by the wish to lead such a life; death by 
the innocent and transitory unconventional act. We also find a story in which 
a woman is reviewing her life and remembering a feeling of excitement and 
then goes back to Italy to die. 

This is an old woman talking of the time when she was young. She's the 
time she's thinking of is when- she first came to America. She came from Italy 
and she's remembering herself and how gkd she was to get here, the whole 
feeling of excitement and life, and she's thinking of the life she's had. At this 
point now, she's on a ship going back to Italy. She's going back to die. 

Thus we see that, in addition to an unconventional life, the memory of 
a feeling of excitement is followed by death. Our hypothesis would then have 



LOVE, SEX, AND MARITAL RELATIONSHIPS 187 

to be that leading an unconventional life or dreaming about it or remembering 
a feeling of excitement may be dangerous. We must look, then for negative 
evidence. Do people who act unconventionally or who are dissatisfied with 
the prosaic, everyday quality of their lives, ever escape death? 
We fend such negative evidence in the following story. 

This man's tired of his life, tired of the common prosaic things; and he's 
looking up into the sky thinking that there is something real because he's 
found nothing real in his life. After a while he closes the window, puts on 
the light in his room, and comes back to things he had to do. Possibly studying, 
possibly he's a student. He forgets pretty easily that he was so discontented 
for a minute. 

How can we explain the fact that this story presents an individual tired 
of the common prosaic things, but for whom there is neither suicide nor 
death? We might first suppose that it is because he "forgets pretty easily that 
he was so discontented for a minute." We have seen before that the girl who 
"learns her lesson and grows up" escapes suicide. It may be the characteristic 
of the mature individual in general. But it may stem from the fact that the 
hero is a man. If this were the case, we should expect to find that, when men 
do what is unconventional, there are no serious consequences; but that when 
women do the same thing, they die. This proved to be the case, as we see in 
the following two stories. 

This girl killed her sister. Her sister is, er, younger than (she). She's always 
been the more attractive and intelligent and the girl who's killing her has 
been jealous. And the climax came when the younger sister stole the other 
girl's boy friend away from her. They had a terrible fight and the younger 
sister is still talking now and saying, *Tou won't kill me"; and finally, the 
older sister stops; she doesn't kill her; and eventually she kills herself. 

This is Jack the Ripper. It's in London and he has with him the heads of the 
bodies inside the bag he's carrying. He's had a very interesting evening! 
Killed a few people here and there, lots of fun. He's going back to his room 
and nail the bodies up on the wall or something. I'm not quite sure what he 
does with them. 

In the first story, the heroine kills her sister, but later decides she cannot 
go through with it, and kills herself. In the second story, however, a man, 
Jack the Ripper, commits murder which results in nothing more than "a very 
interesting evening." Men, then, can either accept the prosaic more easily 
or behave unconventionally with relative impunity. If this difference in the 
sex of the characters is the crucial one, we might expect that this young woman 
would rather be a man; and such is, indeed, the case, as we sec in the 
following story. 

She's sitting in a room looking out of a window and she's watching her 
brothers and their friends outside playing baseball. Itfs spring. She's very proud 



1 88 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

of her brothers but she's also wishing she were a boy and could play with 
them, too. She's about fifteen years old What happens to her is, she reverts 
from her tomboy wishes and aspirations and becomes extremely feminine in a 
couple of years. She sort of realizes she can't be a boy, subconsciously, and 
she decides that, if she can't, at least she wants them to notice her some way 
or other, and she does it by becoming very popular so that all her brothers' 
friends take her out 

The conflict between her wish to be a boy and the knowledge that she 
can never be one is here resolved by her "adoption" of a feminine role. The 
outcome here, as in the story in which she "grows up" is not an altogether 
unhappy one. There is no suggestion of death or suicide. But her preference 
for the male role is indicated by her portrayal of men as either more capable 
of easily throwing off discontent with a prosaic life or more able to commit 
antisocial action without punishment or death. 

SUMMARY OF THE CASE OF NANCY 

From an initial story of a suicide which appeared to be the result of 
rejection in love, we saw that rejection need not lead to suicide if the heroine 
'learns her lesson" and "grows up," and that the heroine who did commit 
suicide comes to the beach "because she wants to dream." From this, we 
hypothesized an antithesis between introversion, with too much emphasis 
placed on dreaming, and extroversion, which is realistic and free of the dangers 
of introversion. We found further evidence that day-dreaming and intro- 
version led to suicide, and that this introversion concerned itself with the 
unconventional life, away from the everyday prosaic life of the heroine. This 
suggested the hypothesis that the antithesis between dreaming and "growing 
up" reflected a conflict between two ways of life the conventional and the 
unconventional that dreaming of the unconventional life, as well as living it, 
was dangerous, and that growing up and leading a more conventional life 
was safe. We found evidence for this hypothesis in the fact that illicit sexual 
relations ended in death for one heroine and that the memory of a feeling 
of excitement in the case of another heroine was followed by death. Negative 
evidence appeared in the story of a man who, discontented with his prosaic 
life, was able easily to forget his discontent. The question arose whether he 
was able to do this because he was a man or because of his maturity. We 
tested these alternatives by looking for differential outcomes for males and 
females committing the same type of antisocial behavior and found that the 
difference in sex was the crucial factor. A man who killed enjoyed himself; 
a woman who killed, or wished to kill, killed herself. We had, therefore, to 
qualify our hypothesis and say that unconventional living or excitement is 
followed by death or suicide only in the case of women, and for this reason 



LOVE, SEX, AND MARITAL RELATIONSHIPS 109 

supposed that this woman would rather be a man. This proved to be the case. 
Consequendy, the dichotomy between the danger which issued from excite- 
ment and unconventional living, or daydreaming about such a life, and the 
safety engendered by a mature, extraverted acceptance of the prosaic life, had 
to be broadened to include a conflict between a masculine, active, and uncon- 
ventional way of life and a feminine, passive, prosaic life. 

We have thus been told why there is a dichotomy between an active, 
exciting, unconventional life and a passive, prosaic way of life. The difference 
is that between the life of a boy and the life of a girl who wanted to remain 
a tomboy. Even in later life, the male is portrayed as the unfettered executive 
of his most antisocial wishes. We have yet to explain why a man may lead 
an exciting life without danger, but a woman dies or commits suicide as a 
consequence of unconventional living or as the consequence of even the wish 
to lead such a life or the feeling of excitement in remembering such a life. 

The answer to this question is not to be found in the TAT. This 
protocol was originally analyzed "blind," without any knowledge of the case, 
and this was as far as the analysis could be carried on the basis of the protocol 
itself. The most crucial factor in this case found no direct reflection in the 
TAT. Nancy had in her childhood been a tomboy, but at the age of 5 con- 
tracted rheumatic fever and was taken to Florida each winter until she was 12. 
She reported that she was not much handicapped by her condition since 
"almost all the kids down there were sick," but that when she came back to 
New York each spring she did notice her restrictions. Her mother never 
told her how serious her condition was, although Nancy understood this from 
the fact that her mother always talked to the doctor alone and tried to keep 
her from overhearing. She has been told to smoke no more than three 
cigarettes a day but ignores this completely. There are limitations on her 
physical activity, however, which she does observe. Thus, she would like 
to take up modern dancing but "won't be allowed to." 

We see, then, that Nancy's fear of the unconventional, active, exciting 
life has a very realistic basis in the possibility of heart failure and that it was 
complicated by her wish to be a tomboy, which was renounced with regret. 
Ir is of considerable significance that this was never mentioned in the TAT, 
although the derivatives of this fear do appear. We have no explanation 
for this, and it is the more puzzling in view of her short stories concerning 
which she tells us, "My short stories used to be very psychological. People 
would be very frightened of something. And they'd end up dying of heart 
failure, or something of that sort." 

We have followed the leads presented by one story through these complex 
interrelationships to illustrate the meaning which may underlie a simple love 
story, and to show that although the protocol may be self illuminating, its 



jpO THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

complete interpretation may call for knowledge which must be sought from 
sources other than the TAT. 

The story o an unhappy love affair revealed in the final analysis that a 
certain way of life may lead to death. This was discovered by an examination 
of the conditions under which love affairs ended in suicide and the conditions 
under which they ended less unhappily. 

Thus suicide in a single story may mean that the conditions in this story 
are dangerous for the individual. In order to prove this it must be demon- 
strated that when the conditions which led to danger in the initial story are 
different in other stories there is safety for the heroes of these other stories. 
In the discussion of multifactor analysis in Chapter IV, we saw that any 
story might be an expression of the individual's central value, threats which 
beset him, invariances characteristic of the individual, or an expression of 
the conditions necessary for any element within the story. 

In further illustration of this method of analysis, let us reconsider the first 
suicide story in the case just discussed, and see what meanings it might have 
had if the other stories in the protocol had been different. 

I. CENTRAL VALUES 

Let us suppose that the story had been an expression of the fact that 
satisfaction of her heterosexual love wish was her central value. If this had 
been the case we would have found other stories in which any threat to her 
love life would have seriously disturbed the heroine. We would have found, 
moreover, that in stories in which there were threats to her career, her family, 
or general social relationships, the heroine was not so disturbed. 

2. THREAT 

If however, the story had been an expression of her vulnerability to any 
threat of loss we would have found other stories in which the heroine suffered 
the loss of friends, parents, or job and reacted in the same suicidal fashion. 

If, on the other hand, the story had been an expression of a general fear 
of any kind of frustration rather than fear of the specific threat of loss we 
would have found other stories which expressed a variety of frustrations all 
resulting in the death of the heroine. 

Or the story might have expressed the individual's differential reaction 
to one type of threat the threat of loss and in that case other types of threat 
would have been more easily tolerated. We would have found other stories 
involving loss which resulted in death for the. heroine, and still others involving 
another type of threat lack of love objects, friends, or parents, for example 
in which the heroine overcame these deficits. 



LOVE, SEX, AND MARI*TAL RELATIONSHIPS 19! 

Or the story might have expressed the individual's differential reaction 
to losses of varying magnitude. If this had been the case there would have 
been other stories involving losses of less serious magnitude which resulted 
in less serious disturbance. It would not be love as such which was crucial 
in this case, but the importance of the particular love object, or the permanency 
of the loss. For example, a lover's quarrel which was later resolved might 
cause distress but not suicide, since the loss of the love object was temporary; 
loss of one love object might be disturbing, whereas the loss of another more 
important love object might cause the heroine to commit suicide. 

3. INVARIANT CHARACTERISTICS 

Let us suppose that the story represented an invariant characteristic of 
the individual. If the story had expressed her invariant expectation that she 
would be disappointed in love, every other love story would have portrayed 
a rejected or frustrated heroine. 

Or it might have expressed the more general expectation that she would 
inevitably be frustrated in all her wishes. If this had been so there would 
have been other stories in which her wishes, no matter what their nature, 
were frustrated. 

It might, however, have been an expression of the fact that she invariably 
withdraws to the level of phantasy when she is frustrated. If this had been so 
there would have been other stories in which the heroine turned unhappily 
to introversion following frustration but was happily extraverted in stories in 
which she could satisfy her wishes. 

The story might have been an expression of a basic self-destructive wish. 
Had this been the case all other stories, whatever else happened, would have 
ended in suicide. 

Had this story represented the invariant reaction of a suicidal wish fol- 
lowing rejection in love, we should have found other love stories in which 
there was no rejection and no suicide, and stories in which she was rejected 
by friends or parents and did not commit suicide. 

4. NECESSARY CONDITIONS 

On the other hand, the story might have been an expression of the neces- 
sary conditions for any element in the story. It might have defined the 
specific conditions which create suicidal wishes in the individual. If this had 
been the case there would have been other stories in which the heroine was 
not rejected and did not commit suicide or in which she was reconciled with 
her love object and did not commit suicide. 



1 92 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

Thus we see that we may determine which of the conditions preceding 
the suicide in our single story, is actually responsible for it, by examining the 
conditions in other stories which do not lead to suicide. 

RANGE OF VARIABILITY 

This is the logic of multifactor analysis. Before it may be applied, how- 
ever, the dimensions of the stories must be identified, and we must then 
determine the extent to which these dimensions vary. There are protocols 
in which the range of variability of any particular dimension is zero and 
others in which the range of variability is very great. The duration of a love 
relationship may be invariant in one protocol all love relationships are eternal 
or transitory; but in other protocols some love affairs are transitory, others are 
of moderate duration, and some are eternal. These differences in the range 
of variability are found in every dimension we have considered. Where the 
range of variability of a dimension is zero, we may consider this presumptive 
evidence that the specific value of this dimension (that all love affairs are 
transitory, for example) represents some invariant characteristic of the indi- 
vidual telling the stories. It may represent his wish, his expectation or his 
behavior. This must be determined in other ways, but whatever the level, 
the invariance of the specific value of the dimension found in the stories 
generally represents something equally stable in the personality of the indi- 
vidual. In some protocols we may have no indication of the specific value 
of a particular dimension. For example, there may be no references whatever 
to the length of time required for the maturation of the relationship. In such 
cases, we would also consider the range of variability to be zero. The actual 
value of this dimension remains indeterminate; but, whatever its value, since 
we find no mention of it we assume that it is a constant in the mind of the 
storyteller. Suppose no outcomes are ever given. We do not know whether 
the individual cannot see an outcome to a love relationship or whether there 
is a typical outcome which is never mentioned; but, in the light of our evi- 
dence, we would assume zero variability for outcomes. 

INTERPRETATION OF VARIABILITY 

Although ttniformity in any dimension is diagnostic of stable elements in 
the personality of the storyteller, variability in any dimension does not neces- 
sarily imply an equivalent variability in the personality. That is, if all stories 
end unhappily, we may assume that there is a particularly rigid expectation 
of unhappy outcomes to any love relationship into which the individual may 
enter. But, if some stories end happily and others end unhappily (with fine 
gradations of happiness and unhappiness in outcomes), we cannot assume 
simply that the individual expects sometimes to be happy and sometimes to be 



LOVE, SEX, AND MARITAL RELATIONSHIPS 193 

sad in the ratio of happy to unhappy endings. Again, if all stories tell of a 
long maturation and the slow ripening of acquaintance into love, we may 
assume that this is the individual's characteristic way of entering into a love 
relationship, or the conditions under which he might enter into a love rela- 
tionship. But, if some of the stories depict hasty marriages and other stories 
tell of childhood sweethearts marrying after years of acquaintance, we clearly 
cannot assume that he will sometimes enter into hasty marriages and some- 
times into more carefully considered relationships in the ratio in which these 
appear in the stories. 

In general, variability which appears in any dimension must be explained 
in one of two ways. It must be shown either that this variability is the effect 
of differences in preceding conditions if in two or more stories or that these 
differences produce different effects in the stories into which they are intro- 
duced, or both. 

For example, consider the following two stories. 

These pepole work together in the same place. At first, he didn't pay much 
attention to her. But she was very kind and considerate and one day she 
offered to stay after work and help him finish his work. This makes a big 
impression on him. He accepts and afterwards he notices more and more 
how she always goes out of her way to do nice things for him and he comes 
to love her deeply. 

This man and woman have known each other for a long time. They like to 
do things together, like going to plays and concerts together. They have a lot 
in common, and lie likes her a lot They will always be very good friends. 

In these stories, the variability in intensity between "he comes to love her 
deeply" and "he likes her a lot,'* are effects of two different conditions. We 
may infer from these two stories that women who nurture the hero elicit 
intense feelings of love, and that women who are companionable elicit less 
intense feelings. We cannot be certain either that he actually has two such 
relationships or would wish to. We know only that different kinds of 
behavior on the part of women elicit variations in the intensity of his feelings. 

In the stories which follow, variations of intensity of the hero's feelings 
produce variations in the outcome of the relationship. 

This man loves her desperately. He has never before been so completely swept 
off his feet. He has just finished telling her of his feelings. She tells him 
that she likes him, but she can't marry him. He feels that she is laughing 
at him. 

This is a picture of a proposal. He has just told her that she means a great 
deal to him and asked her to marry him. She says she will and they're sealing 
their 1 engagement with a kiss. 



194 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TOST 

In these two stories, we are told that, if the hero's feelings of love are 
too intense, they will not be returned in kind and that he will interpret this 
as rejection; but, when his feelings are less intense, they may be reciprocated. 

Utilizing this type of analysis, complete interpretation of each region 
requires an explanation of the variability of those dimensions which exhibit 
any degree of variability as well as an examination of dimensions which are 
invariant. And, finally, these two sets of dimensions must be examined for 
their interrelationships. 

Each of the dimensions illustrated in the love, marital, and sex region is 
capable of variation which is either the effect of variation in another dimension 
or the cause of such variation. Actually, such variability is not infrequendy 
the resultant of the concomitant variation of a number of other dimensions. 
But were we to restrict our analysis to those cases in which the variability 
was a function of only one other dimension, we would find that the inter- 
relationships between the definition of the situation, reciprocity, intensity, 
temporal characteristics, source of disruption, maintenance or disruption with- 
out further specification would number thirty. In any one protocol, however, 
the important functional relationships are usually simple and limited. But if 
one considers the population of all TAT protocols, the actual variety of such 
functional relationships, while finite, is extremely great, since specific aspects 
of each dimension may vary independently and a dimension may vary as a 
function of the variability of sets of dimensions. 

Documentation of these varieties of functional relationships would require 
encyclopedic treatment beyond the scope of this book, but we shall present 
some of the more common and more simple of these functional relationships 
which have been found in the analysis of a variety of TAT protocols. These 
examples are, however, but a sample of even the most common of these 
relationships. 

A. DEFINITION OF THE SITUATION 

i. As a Cause 

As a dimension responsible for variation in any other dimension, some 
common resultants are the following: a) Reciprocity. The hero reciprocates 
nurturant love and rejects dependent love. The hero reciprocates the love of 
companionable love objects and rejects domineering love objects. The love 
object rejects the love of a dependent hero and reciprocates the love of a 
nurturant hero. 

b) Intensity. The hero feels strongly toward nurturant love objects and 
less so toward companionable love objects. He has little intensity of feeling 
toward love objects he possesses and feels more strongly toward love objects 
who are aloof. The hero's feelings toward a beautiful love object are intense, 



LOVE, SEX, AND MARITAL RELATIONSHIPS 195 

and less intense toward a love object o less beauty, or he has intense feelings 
toward a beautiful love object and less intense Eeelings toward a nurturant love 
object. The love object responds to the hero's dominance with intense love 
and to his nurturance with less intense love. 

c) Temporal characteristics. Marriages based on nurturance and depend- 
ence endure while those based on companionship are short-lived. Relation- 
ships based on dominance and submission endure while those based on the 
esthetic quality of the love object do not endure. 

d) Sources of disruption. Relationships based on the esthetic quality of 
the love object are subject to disruption from exogenous sources, while rela- 
tionships based on companionship are not subject to disruption from exogenous 
sources. Relationships based on submission to dominance result in disruption 
from endogenous sources while those based on dependence do not. 

e) Maintenance or disruption. If disruption is suffered, the type of rela- 
tionship determines whether reconciliation is possible. Thus, a relationship 
based on companionship is reconciled while a relationship based on possession 
of the loved object can not be reconciled. When the love object is submissive 
to the hero the relationship is reconciled but when the love object dominates 
the hero the relationship cannot be reconciled. 

Where the relationship is based on possession of the love object, finding 
another love object may be impossible, but where it is based on esthetic enjoy- 
ment of the love object, the hero finds someone else he can love. Where the 
relationship is based on companionship, the hero is able to find satisfaction in 
another region; but where the hero dominates the love object this is not 
possible. 

2. Definition of the Situation as Effect 

a) Reciprocity. If his love is reciprocated, the hero wishes to take care of 
his love object; but if his love is not reciprocated, he wishes to dominate the 
love object. If the hero is uncertain of the reciprocity of love he wishes to be 
dependent; when love is reciprocated then the wish is to take care of the 
love object. 

b) Intensity. If the hero's wishes are not very intense he desires no more 
than companionship; when his wishes are more intense he desires another type 
of relationship, e.g., to be possessive* 

c) Temporal characteristics. If a relationship has existed for a long period 
of time, then sexual relations suddenly become possible; whereas if the rela- 
tionship has existed for only a short period of time, the hero does not have 
sexual relations. 

d) Sources of disruption. An exogenous disrupter changes a relationship 
from one of dependence fo one of possession; but if there is no exogenous 
disrupter it remains a dependent relationship. 



196 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

e) Maintenance or disruption. Reconciliation of one type changes a rela- 
tionship from a possessive one to one based on companionship, whereas 
reconciliation of another type leaves the possessive relationship unchanged. 
If there is no disruption there is no change in the relationship; if there is 
disruption, however, there is a change in the kind of relationship desired 
by the hero. 

B. RECIPROCITY 
i. As a Cause 

a) Intensity. The reciprocity of love increases the intensity of the hero's 
feelings of love; lack of reciprocity decreases their intensity. 

b) Temporal characteristics. Reciprocated love produces an enduring rela- 
tionship. If love is not reciprocated the hero's feelings do not endure. 

c) Sources of disruption. Unreciprocated love leaves the individual open 
to exogenous sources of disruption. Reciprocated love, on the other hand, 
leaves the individual open to endogenous sources of disruption. 

d) Maintenance or disruption. Unreciprocated love allows no reconcilia- 
tion in the event of disruption of the relationship. Reciprocated love allows 
reconciliation after disruption. 

2. As an Effect 

a) Intensity. The more intense the feelings of the hero, the more they 
a're reciprocated. If the hero's feelings are less intense they are not reciprocated. 

b) Temporal characteristics. In enduring relationships the hero's feelings 
are not altogether reciprocated. In relationships which are transitory the hero 
docs not altogether reciprocate the feelings of the love object. Slow matura- 
tion results in reciprocity on the part of the love object; fast maturation results 
in a lack of reciprocity. 

c) Sources of disruption. Exogenous disruption leads to reciprocity of love 
afterwards, Endogenous disruption leads to the hero's unreciprocated wish 
for the love object. 

d) Maintenance or disruption. Reconciliation leads to a new reciprocity 
of love; no reconciliation leads to the hero's unreciprocated wish for the love 
object. 

C. INTENSITY 

i. As a Cause 

a) Temporal characteristics. Intense love relationships do not last; less 
intense relationships are more enduring. 

b) Sources of disruption. Intensity of feeling on the part of the hero 



LOVE, SEX, AND MARITAL RELATIONSHIPS Ip7 

leads to endogenous disruption. Less intense feelings on the part of the hero 
lead to exogenous disruption. 

c) Maintenance or disruption. Intense feelings lead to reconciliation in 
the event of disruption; less intense feelings lead to no attempt at reconciliation. 

2. As an Effect 

a) Temporal characteristics. If a relationship has endured, it results in 
increased intensity of feeling; if the relationship has existed for only a short 
while, feeling is less intense. 

b) Sources of disruption. If the disruption arises from exogenous sources, 
the hero's feelings decrease in intensity; if the disruption arises from endoge- 
nous sources, the hero's feelings increase in intensity. 

c) Maintenance or disruption. If there is reconciliation after disruption, 
feelings decrease in intensity; if there is no reconciliation, feelings increase in 
intensity. 

D. TEMPORAL CHARACTERISTICS 

i. As a Cause 

a) Sources of disruption. If the relationship has matured slowly, it is 
open to disruption from exogenous sources; if the relationship has matured 
rapidly it is open to disruption from endogenous sources. 

b) Maintenance or disruption. If the relationship has been long-enduring, 
reconciliation is possible; if it has existed for only a short time and there is 
disruption, reconciliation is impossible. 

2. As an Effect 

a) Sources of disruption. If the relationship has been disrupted from 
exogenous sources (the death of the love object), the relationship is preserved 
in the memory of the hero. If the relationship has been disrupted by endoge- 
nous forces, the relationship does not endure, and neither the hero nor his love 
object wish that it had endured. 

b) Maintenance or disruption. If the relationship is reconciled it endures, 
but, if it is disrupted and there is no attempt at reconciliation, it does not 
endure. 

E. SOURCES OF DISRUPTION 

i. As a Cause 

a) Maintenance or disruption. If disruption arises from exogenous 
sources, the lovers are reconciled; if it arises from endogenous sources, no 
reconciliation is possible. 



198 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

2, As an Effect 

a) Maintenance or disruption. If a relationship has suffered disruption 
and has then been reconciled, it is open to further disruption from endogenous 
sources. 

We have, in these samples of functional interrelationships, considered 
primarily the variations in specific values of the dimensions of the love region. 
The same type of analysis may be applied to the sex region. Further, the same 
type of analysis may be applied comparing the variability of dimensions in 
both love and sex. For example, it may be asked whether the definitions of 
the love and sexual situations are the same, or different; and if they differ, 
in what direction and for what reason. It may further be asked whether there 
are differences in the duration, the relative intensity, liability to disruption, 
and potentiality of regeneration. 

In these illustrations of the functional relationships between dimensions 
of the love region, we have excluded from consideration all the interrelation- 
ships which may and do exist between this region and other regions and 
between this region and the personality structure of the individual whose 
activity in this limited region has been isolated from the larger matrix. 

SUMMARY 

In Part I we considered those dimensions of the love and marital region 
important to the individual's adjustment in this area. We first examined the 
nature of the relationship, inquiring into the individual's definition of the love 
relationship. It was shown that different individuals are in quest of diverse 
gratifications in their love life. Some of the more common of these are wishes 
to be dependent, to care for the love object, to govern or be governed, to 
possess the love object, to share intimacies, or derive esthetic enjoyment from 
the love object. 

We next considered the degree of reciprocity in the love relationship, 
illustrating relationships which represented the middle and end points of a 
continuum ranging from complete mutuality to complete lack of reciprocity. 

The intensity of the feeling of love was seen to be independent of the 
nature of the love relationship and the extent to which it might be reciprocated. 

We next examined the temporal dimension, illustrating differences in the 
rate of maturation of the love relationship and the length of time the relation- 
ships endure. We said that the relationship might outlast the feelings of love, 
or the feelings continue after the relationship is broken; that the feelings might 
continue only as long as the relationship was maintained or the relationship 
might be maintained only as long as the feelings of love continued. 

We then inquired into the vicissitudes of the love relationship, examining 



LOVE, SEX, AND MARITAL RELATIONSHIPS 199 

first the sources of disruption. There were seen to be threats either exogenous 
or endogenous in origin. We then considered the extent to which these 
threats might disrupt the relationship. We saw that if these threats were 
successfully met this might be achieved through a common sorrow, or reforma- 
tion, or the hero's new appreciation of the qualities* of 'the love object, or 
through the acceptance of a lower love income. When the relationship is 
completely disrupted the individual may never relinquish the possibility of 
future reconciliation or may seek symbolic reunion in death. The disruption 
of the love relationship may result in further disruption of the individual's 
life, or he may seek another love object, or satisfaction in some other region. 
These attempts may or may not be successful. Another sequel of a disrupted 
love relationship is aggression directed either against the love object or the 
self. Suicide was seen to be an instrumental act providing an escape from 
inner misery rather than an expression of a wish to destroy the self. 

In Part II we examined the dimensions of the sex region, since there is, 
in our society, sufficient discrepancy in the expression of love and sex to justify 
a separate scrutiny of the dimensions of these two regions. In general the 
same dimensions were found useful in both regions. But although every type 
of love relationship has its sexual counterpart, the "individual may define the 
love relationship quite differently from the sexual relationship. Thus an indi- 
vidual may seek gratification of his dependency wish in love or marriage, but 
wish to dominate his sexual partner. We then examined stories for evidence 
of homosexual wishes. We distinguished identification with the opposite sex, 
wish to be of opposite sex, and love objects of the opposite sex. We saw that 
these were independent variables, that whether one loves members of the 
same or the opposite sex is .not determined either by identification with same 
or the opposite sex or by the wish to be of the opposite sex or one's own sex. 

We then examined the degree of reciprocity in the sexual relationship and 
the intensity and the temporal characteristics of the sex need. 

Next we discussed the vicissitudes of the sexual relationship. We noted 
that although the heroes of TAT stories are concerned with the duration of 
the love relationship, it was a very exceptional hero who was anxious about 
the duration of a strictly sexual relationship, and that although the hero's life 
might be shattered by the disruption of a love relationship, there are no such 
sequelae to the disruption of a sexual relationship, nor are the poignant attempts 
to reconstitute a disrupted love relationship ever paralleled in the quest of a 
lost sex object. But if the sex need is taken less seriously in this respect, it is 
nonetheless beset with anxieties peculiar to the particular pressures of American 
society. We examined first the varieties of degree of inhibition of sexual 
behavior, ranging from the individual who is incapable of experiencing sexual 
feeling to those who may consummate the sexual act but with acute remorse 



200 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

or anxiety thereafter. Less frequently in TAT stories does the hero consum- 
mate the sexual act with enjoyment and no remorse or fear. We saw that the 
sources of this inhibition might be exogenous or endogenous. 

In Part III we undertook the interpretation of these dimensions of the 
love, marital, and sex regions. We examined one protocol in some detail to 
illustrate the dependence of interpretation on the pattern of the whole protocol, 
and in this case on information which was not revealed in the TAT. We 
examined again the multiplicity of meanings which any single story may 
express, using the techniques of multifactor analysis outlined in Chapter IV. 
We saw that the meaning of a single love story of a girl who takes her life, 
depended entirely on the concomitant variation between elements of this 
story, and other stories. We said further that the dimensions of any region 
must first be described in terms of their range of variability. Those dimen- 
sions which were invariant were assumed to represent stable elements in the 
personality of the individual telling the story. Those dimensions which varied 
throughout the protocols had to be explained either as the cause of other 
effects in the stories in which they were introduced, or as the effects of other 
causes. Finally, Li interpreting the protocol, the invariant and variable dimen- 
sions had to be related to each other. 

We examined systematically some of the more common functional rela- 
tionships between dimensions of the love region. We excluded from considera- 
tion all the interrelationships which may and do exist between this region 
and other regions, and this region and the personality structure of the indi- 
vidual whose activity in this limited region has been isolated from the larger 
matrix. 



CHAPTER VIII 

DIAGNOSIS OF PERSONALITY: THE REGION OF 
SOCIAL RELATIONSHIPS 

Every region thus far considered involves social relationships to some 
extent: interaction between parent and child, between employer and worker, 
and between lovers all constitute social relationships. But since these have 
been considered elsewhere, only the residuum will be the topic of this chapter. 
We have further limited the scope of our inquiry, even within this narrowly 
conceived sphere, to antisocial behavior. We have focused on this sector of 
social relationships in view of the importance of the control of antisocial wishes 
in the socialization of the individual and the difficulties inherent in investi- 
gating wishes which the normally socialized individual usually guards from 
public scrutiny. As we have seen before, the TAT is peculiarly sensitive in 
exposing antisocial wishes which are deeply imbedded in the recesses of the 
personality. 

Part I will be devoted to the illustration of dimensions of antisocial 
behavior, and Part II will be devoted to the interpretation of these dimensions 

I. THE DIMENSIONS OF ANTISOCIAL BEHAVIOR 
A. THE FORM OF ANTISOCIAL BEHAVIOR 

We must first ask what form the antisocial behavior takes. The behavior 
may take the form of aggression (vector "against"), as in the following story, 
in which the hero kills his best friend. 

The man has been jealous of his best friend and kills him. He goes to his 
grave begging forgiveness and drops dead over the tombstone. 

Or it may take the form of acquisitiveness (vector "from"), as in this next 
story in which the hero, who is a spendthrift, steals to supplement his salary. 

We'll call this one *7 ust ice." Reynolds had lived a careless life. He had never 
felt the importance of duty and responsibility he owed to himself and others. 
He had been primarily concerned with his own pleasure. As a young man, 
he became a spendthrift and very soon the small salary he earned as a clerk 
in a brokerage firm did not suffice to supply him with his wants. The firm 
had hired Reynolds as a gesture o appreciation to his father who had worked 
for it previously. Before many months passed, questionable items began to 
appear on his accounts. Becoming suspicious, the owners of the firm warned 
Reynolds, then proceeded to watch him. Unfortunately for him, having spent 
all his week's wages in a nearby tavern, on gambling and drink, Reynolds 



202 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

decided to return to his office to replenish his cash out of the firm's safe whose 
combination he knew. Upon opening the safe, at this late hour, without 
authority, he was apprehended by several detectives who laid hands upon 
him and took him into custody. He was found guilty of attempted embezzle- 
ment and would have been jailed for a long time had not his employers pleaded 
leniency for him in the hope that in some other position and with this 
experience as a bitter lesson, he would make good. There is certain justice 
following the deeds of men. 

Or it may take form of desertion (vector "away from"). 

Might be a soldier who deserted. He never believed in war anyhow and he 
thought his own skin was more important than a lot of flag waving by a 
bunch of war profiteers. So he just upped and walked out, got himself some 
civilian clothes and went about his business. Some MPs have caught up with 
him and are taking him to the guard house. He'll probably be court 
marshalled. 

Or the antisocial behavior may take the form of dominance (vector 
"over"), as in the story which follows: a hypnotist who uses his power to 
make his subject do things against his basic nature. 

A hypnotist is trying his practice out on a subject. He has in mind to perfect 
his technique beyond that of normal hypnotists that is to a degree where he 
can command the subject to do things that are against the subject's basic nature. 
When he thinks that he has his technique worked out he will try it on someone 
who will come out of the spell and in a spell of anger hurt the hypnotist or 
threaten him with the law (poetic justice). 

B. MOTIVATION OF ANTISOCIAL BEHAVIOR 

What are the conditions that motivate such behavior? In the story which 
we again reproduce, jealousy (vector "from") incites aggression. 

The man has been jealous of his best friend and kills him. He goes to his 
grave begging forgiveness and drops dead over the tombstone. 

Antisocial behavior may also be motivated by the wish to help someone 
(vector "for") arising from a previous poverty (condition lack), as in the 
following rescue phantasy. 

The boy has just told his mother about committing a robbery. Before they 
were very poor and he decided to give his mother luxuries in her last years. 
He robbed a bank and told his mother about it She is unhappy but tells her 
son to give himself up. The son does this and spends the next ten years in jail. 

Antisocial behavior may also be motivated by the desire to be like others 
(level-wish-object, condition abundance) arising from poverty (condition lack). 

Joe didn't have much education. He lived on, the other side of the tracks, and 
his friends were limited. He used to love to play Cops and Robbers. His 



DIAGNOSIS OF PERSONALITY: SOCIAL RELATIONSHIPS 203 

parents didn't care much about him, and they were very poor. Joe was very 
self conscious about his feeble clothes, he wanted to be like other boys, and it 
was in this way he got tangled up in a murder situation, and was forced into 
committing a crime. We find him in this picture just after the crime has 
been committed. He knows he's done wrong, and is upset. 

Or there may be a wish to get what other people have (vector "from") 
arising from poverty (condition lack) and the hostility it arouses (vector 
"against"). 

Dave had been brought up in the slums of one of our big cities and received 
his education from his own imagination. He thought he was wronged in being 
born in poverty and always bore this grudge against the world. Instead of 
making an honest success of himself, he strove to get what he wanted and 
what other people had through crime. He became brutal and hard. Til one 
day, upon escaping in a stolen car, he chose to run down an elderly woman 
rather than risk smashing up. She was an old scrubwoman, mother of seven 
children, whom she slaved to bring up right and turned out to be an old 
neighbor of his. When later caught and faced in the morgue by the mangled 
body, he could not bear it and screeched for mercy before collapsing. He 
later died, coward as he was, in the electric chair, still with a distorted mind. 
But we were supposed to regard our lives as luxuries rather than sacrifice, 

Or the inciter may be a wish which the hero must obey (vector "under," 
object self). 

This old servant has been caught stealing food out of the pantry. He is a 
well-fed servant but he feels he must steal and not desiring to steal something 
valuable he steals food. This inner urge has gotten him into trouble before 
in the same manner, and this time he is fired. He has no place to go and he 
is turned out until weak with hunger he goes to the poorhouse. 

An antisocial act may be caused by the hero's behavior itself, which passes 
beyond the control of the hero (vector "under," object behavior-object self). 

The woman facing us has been showing the other woman some neck holds. 
Suddenly she lost control, as is shown, and began really to choke her. The 
woman lost consciousness and at that point the woman facing us gets a grip 
on herself, and stopped choking. Immediately afterward she has hysterics. The 
other woman tries to show her that everything is all right, but she is really 
afraid, and their friendship will be broken by fear on the one hand and 
self-mistrust on the other. 

Or antisocial behavior may be the consequence of drinking (Special State). 

The man shown is drunk. He's a perfectly respectable citizen when he is 
sober, but he's a terror when he's drunk. He gets mean and wants to fight 
everybody, and doesn't quite know what he is doing. He was in a bar and 
had had a few too many and he started a fight with the guy standing next 
to him and there was a free-for-all, and in this free-for-all he hit the guy over 



204 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

the head with a bottle and killed him. He's being led away by the police here 
and he's still too drunk to know what happened. When he wakes up in the 
morning and finds himself facing a murder rap he's going to be stunned. 

Or it may be incited by feelings of inferiority (feeling-object condition 
lack). 

She's a hunchback and her deformity has affected her life. It made her a 
very bitter, malevolent person because she feels inferior to other people, and 
feels that they are always looking at her and pitying her. She comes to hate 
people, particularly young girls her own age, who are going out and enjoying 
themselves in a way that's impossible for her. A friend of hers came to see 
her, not to gloat or to pity her as she thought but because she really wanted 
to see her. They were sitting talking when she was suddenly seized with a 
burning hatred, all the years of feeling inferior and hating burst forth, she 
scarcely knew what she was doing and she picked up the nearest thing she 
could lay her hands on and came toward this girl with her arm upraised 
ready to crush her skull. The girl saw her coining and ran screaming from 
the room. The hunchback collapsed on the floor realizing what she had almost 
done, she was afraid. She started to cry, it really didn't matter what happened 
to her she knew that she could never be happy. 

Or it may have its inception in a growing hatred (vector "against") arising 
from submission (vector u under") to extortion (vector "from"). 

The wicked looking old woman in the background is a blackmailer. Years 
ago she found out something about the younger woman and since then she 
has demanded money or she says she will tell the woman*s husband. The 
young woman, despises her but she has had to pay the money, there's not much 
else she could do. She has stood it for years and is fast reaching the point 
where she can't stand it. She gets so she hates the old woman more and more 
and the next time she comes for her money the young woman pulls out a 
gun and shoots her. She feels no regret, the old woman was a parasite on 
society and shouldn't be allowed to live. The old woman has no friends and 
if she disposes of the body no one will ever wonder what happened to her. 
She disposes of the body and lives in peace after that. 

Or the antisocial behavior may be a consequence of rejection by others 
(vector "against") which incited counteraggression (vector "against"). 

The story of a young man who has first been apprehended for having com- 
mitted robbery and later many murders. Being presented with the picture 
taken of him just after one crime he was forced by police to face the camera 
begins suddenly to realize that there was more behind this picture than might 
appear to anyone else. It might help in the solitude of his own imaginings, and 
explain them as such to a visiting prison psychologist, that his hands reminded 
him of constant clutching that was going on in hi life pulling him back into 
crime whenever he tried to break away from it He told him how in early 
life he had been thwarted and many mixed-up circumstances ... to his com- 
plete self-satisfaction. That constant rebuffs which he received in early life so 



DIAGNOSIS OF PERSONALITY : SOCIAL RELATIONSHIPS 205 

sensitized him against society that he could never really appreciate it These 
rebuffs he experienced in early childhood ... his entrance into his own male 
world of friends, since they considered him a spoiled child and taunted him 
with their dislike, and instead of trying to overcome it, he let it affect him and 
thus this fear of rebuff had a grip on him, prohibiting him from a normal 
young life. When he came into age of entrance in mixed society, there was 
shown the same regard towards women, no, by women, because by that time 
he had developed such a cynical personality, something which was innate 
although not totally deserved by him. This situation affected him, so he had 
eventually turned from society and turned to crime and a revengeful attitude. 
Whenever he did stop to realize what he was doing, and would try to make 
an attempt to turn away from these fears, inhibitions, and repressions, his 
sensitive feelings seemed to grip him, and pull him back and say, "Stay away 
don't want them," till, finally, one crime led to another. The result was 
ultimate apprehension, and while waiting to die in the electric chair, he 
donated his time fully to psychical research, and social writings for the benefit 
of any other starting out with the same predicament, and after death donated 
his body to a nearby medical school, showing his innate liking for society 
although overshadowed by a superficial distaste for it 



C. DIRECTION OF ANTISOCIAL BEHAVIOR 

Against whom is the antisocial behavior directed? It may be directed 
against an individual. 

The young man had gone through a trying mental period such as Dostoievski 
might describe. He finally decided to murder an acquaintance. The thought 
tormented him, seared his brain, yet was not entirely unpleasant to him. He 
laid out the details of the murder carefully, but when it came time for the 
actual deed, he stood before the sleeping victim who was lying on the couch 
and couldn't shoot The victim awoke went screaming out of the room, the 
man realized he had failed due to weakness he hadn't counted on, and realizing 
that there is no use fleeing, he shot himself as shown in the picture. No future. 

Or it may be directed against some particular class or group. 

This guy is a Marxist He's in some South American country .trying to start 
a revolution. Things are pretty well organized. They're out to get the 
capitalists and to persuade the proletariat to join them. Everything goes pretty 
well, the revolutionists run through the street firing capitalistic establishments 
and yelling *T>own with the capitalists, the revolution has come." But the 
capitalists get the soldiers after them and this guy, the leader, just manages to 
escape by climbing this rope over the side of the ship. They're disheartened 
that they've failed but are determined to try again. 

Or it may be directed against the social order. 

The man is an instigator of a revolt Having laid his plans he now has gone 
home and standing at the window with the room darkened he watches the 



206 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

explosion in a gov't building which is a signal for the revolt to begin. 
Mingled emotions are experienced by him at this instant, fear for an instant, 
then excitement and joy trust of his companions. 



D. THE ROLE OF THE HERO IN ANTISOCIAL BEHAVIOR 

I. THE INSTIGATOR 

Who instigates the behavior? It may be instigated by the hero. 

Jimmy was a notorious little prankster around the neighborhood. He was 
regarded by all the storekeepers and pushcart peddlers and other commercial 
institutions around the slum district in which he lived as a general nuisance. 
He stole whenever possible and upset ashcans and was generally being chased 
by the police for some prank or misdeed of some kind or another. Jimmy had 
a gang. The gang's reputation, if anything was worse than, than Jimmy's and 
together they pulled off some of the best bits of small sized larceny, small scale, 
larceny that any group of school boys could ever be, could ever hope to do, ah, 
however, on this particular occasion they were engaged in breaking into a 
candy store one evening about ten o'clock and they were apprehended and 
rushed off to the nearest police station. Jimmy's father a hard-working truck 
driver, was quite upset when he heard about this and rushed down to the 
jail heard Jimmy's story, Jimmy was full of tears and protests and sobbed that 
he would reform absolutely if dad would only see to it that they didn't take 
him away and put him in the reformatory and I guess we'd best round it off 
by having him put away, or rather not put away but let loose and returning 
to his old existence of small scale robbery and such like troubles. 

Or the hero may instigate the behavior and persuade others to join him. 

This guy got a bad start in life. He was kinda smart alecky when he was in 
school and he wasn't in school as he should have been. I mean he didn't go 
much. He thought he was smart. He formed a gang, told the other kids 
that they weren't smart, that they should get smart and not waste their time 
in school either. They formed a gang and he was the leader. At first they 
didn't do much but run around and get in trouble, they were hoodlums and 
got in trouble for not being in school and for breaking windows. But this 
guy who was the leader got big ideas and thought maybe they'd swipe some 
stuff". The other kids weren't for it, but he was the leader and he talked to 
them and told them to be smart. They started swiping things and the things 
got more and more, the things they swiped. When they got older they were 
regular crooks and the police were on the watch for them. One night they 
planned to rob a bank but what they didn't know was that somebody had 
tipped the police off that they were going to do it and they were waiting for 
them. There was a lot of shooting and the leader and some other guys were 
shot up and the police wagon came down the street and took them off to jail. 

Or someone else may persuade the hero to undertake antisocial behavior. 

This is the story of a weak minded man who had no will power, he let a 
friend of his talk him into taking money from his firm to bet on the horses. 



DIAGNOSIS OF PERSONALITY: SOCIAL RELATIONSHIPS 207 

The friend kept on saying they would win and he could pay it back. He was 
weak minded, had no self control so he let himself be talked into it knowing 
all the time that maybe he couldn't pay it back. They won some money, not 
very much but some and he wanted to pay the money back but the friend 
said they needed the money to make more money and after that they started to 
lose and didn't have enough to pay it back and they went on betting until 
they lost it all. The friend left the man to hold the bag and went off to find 
another sucker. The man realizes that he's never going to be able to get that 
money and he doesn't know what to do. He goes home and puts a bullet 
through his head. 

Or someone may coerce the hero leading him into a life of crime. 

Some bandits had been kidnapping boys for their own use. One boy, David 
Johnson, was brought to the chief who taught him many tricks and changed 
him from a weakling to a strong boy. He gave him a gun so he could practice 
shooting, robbing, and killing. In his younger days, he didn't like to fool 
with guns, but his master had told him to, so he had to. At first, the jobs 
were simple hold-ups. Then robbing banks. Then kidnapping and sabotag- 
ing. But this was not to be slightly disregarded by the police; they had found 
that the ringleader had kidnapped the boys who robbed and killed, not because 
they wanted to but because they were made to. One time, the boy David began a 
hold-up of a bank with fifty thousand grand. At first, he killed the cashier, 
took the money and went by the couch, but was intercepted by the police. His 
gun fired but his shot went high. Sharpshooters of the police found their 
mark. He slowly and defiantly slumped to the floor. This is one of the awful 
ways in which to die. 



2. THE SOCIAL CONTEXT 

Is the antisocial act done by the hero in the company of others, or does 
he act alone? The hero may have an ally. 

Steve and Jim has been pals together back in Brooklyn when they were kids. 
They hated school and hardly ever went, they took to hanging around pool 
rooms instead and living how you pleased. They hear the men in the pool 
rooms talking about easy ways to make money and decide to try their hand at 
it. They start stealing cars. Steve drives them while Jim* watches for the 
cops. It was fun and exciting even though a little risky at times. They sell 
the cars and for the present they haven't a care in the world and they aren't 
worrying about the future. One night Jim sees a cop coming and warns 
Steve to get away in a hurry but it* s too late. Steve gets shot trying to get 
away and Jim ends up where most lawbreakers do. 

Or he may be a member of a gang. 

Hm, we got a, here we have a guy who's a member of a gang of crooks, a 
dope ring. They have men who smuggle the stuff in for them and they sell 
it in this country, peddle it. This woman who's his girl has heard that the 
FBI is on their trail and she's trying to get him to get out of it before they 



get caught. She's trying to make him promise but he's trying to get away 
from her to warn the others. She won't let him go until he promises so 
he does. 

Docs the hero regard himself as a member of an in-group or out-group 
in his antisocial behavior? In the story that follows the criminal has an out- 
group feeling. In his activities, he is opposed to the rest of society, which 
constitutes an in-group. 

The story of a young man who has first been apprehended for having com- 
mitted robbery and later many murders. Being presented with the picture 
taken of him just after one crime he was forced by police to face the camera 
begins suddenly to realize that there was more behind this picture than might 
appear to anyone else. It might help in the solitude of his own imaginings, and 
explain them as such to a visiting prison psychologist, that his hands reminded 
him of constant clutching that was going on in his own life pulling him back 
into crime whenever he tried to break away from it. He told him how in 
early life he had been thwarted and many mixed-up circumstances ... to 
his complete self-satisfaction. That constant rebuffs which he received early 
in life so sensitized him against society that he could never really appreciate 
it. These rebuffs he experienced in early childhood ... his entrance into his 
own male world of friends, since they considered him a spoiled child and 
taunted him with their dislike, and instead of trying to overcome it, he let it 
affect him and thus this fear of rebuff had a grip on him, prohibiting him 
from a normal young life. When he came into age of entrance in mixed 
society, there was shown the same regard towards women, no, by women, 
because by that time he had developed such a cynical personality, something 
which was innate although not totally deserved by him. This situation affected 
him, so he had eventually turned from society and turned to crime and a 
revengeful attitude. Whenever he did stop to realize what he was doing, and 
would try to make an attempt to turn away from these fears, inhibitions, and 
repression, his sensitive feelings seemed to grip him, and pull him back and say, 
"Stay away don't want them," till, finally, one crime led to another. The 
result was ultimate apprehension, and while waiting to die in the electric chair, 
he donated his time to psychical research, and social writings for the benefit 
of any other starting out with the same predicament, and after death donated 
his body to a nearby medical school, showing his innate liking for society 
although overshadowed by superficial distaste for it. 

This story illustrates both an out-group feeling on the part of the criminal 
and a later wish to be a part of the in-group. It was the frustration of this wish 
which causecj him to become a criminal. 

In the following there is an in-group feeling amongst criminals who are 
a closely knit group, loyal only to each other and opposed to the rest of society 
as an out-group. There is only for an "instant" a feeling that society is the 
larger in-group. 

The man in this story is a member of a gang of criminals. They are carefully 
organized and they pride themselves that there are brains behind their activities* 



DIAGNOSIS OF PERSONALITY: SOCIAL RELATIONSHIPS 209 

Every operation is carefully conceived and cleverly carried out No member 
has ever been caught. The only loyalty they know is to each other and it is a 
practical kind, a bond of self preservation. They are linked together by the 
knowledge that a chain is only as strong as its weakest link. This night one of 
the men has been placed as sentry to wait by a lighted lamp post for the 
policeman who walks the beat. He is there to be seen by the policeman and 
then with the greatest speed he is to be whisked to another part of town far 
away to commit a crime. The object of tonight's operation is a great loot of 
money. It will be necessary to kill a man but murder has never deterred this 
crowd. These men know no fear, but just for an instant, as the man stands 
by the post, carrying out the first step of the plan, he almost feels that through 
the fog, eyes are peering at him and civilized mankind is learning the guilty 
secret. Soon the foot steps of the policeman are heard and he sighs with 
relief. He never liked waiting. Killing, stealing, tricking, these things never 
bothered him, but waiting if he ever made a misstep, he knew it would be 
when he was just waiting. The policeman left, the plan rolled on like clock 
work. It was his gun that shot the man, but he felt no remorse, no regrets. 



3. THE ACCEPTANCE OF RESPONSIBILITY 

Does the hero assume responsibility for the act? He may feel that he is 
really innocent. 

Hunger made this man steal some food. He felt that he hadn't done wrong, 
that the world owed a man enough to eat. But the manager of the store and 
the police don't agree with him and he goes to jail for thirty days. 

Or that he is not responsible for his behavior. 

He's a madman. Strange spells come over him when he doesn't know what 
he's doing. Some nights he comes out of these spells to find himself wandering 
through dark and desolate sections of the city, places he's never seen before. 
In his sane moments he's haunted by the fear of what he might do when in 
one of these spells. He tries to lock himself in, but he always manages to get out. 
He reads in the newspaper aoout a series of murders in the section of town 
near the wharfs where he had found himself on several occasions. Knowing 
that he isn't responsible for what he does when he's in a spell he's frightened 
and thinks that he might be the murderer. No matter whether it's true or 
not he has to know. He rushes over to look at the clothes he wore the night 
before and is horrified to see deep blood stains on them. He drops to his knees 
trying to think. He wonders if he should give himself up, if he told anyone 
about these spells they either wouldn't believe him or they would say he was 
insane and put him in an institution. The thought terrifies him and he 
commits suicide. 

Or that he was intoxicated and didn't know what he was doing. 

The man shown is drunk. He's a perfect respectable citizen when sober, but 
he's a terror when he's drunk. He gets mean and wants to fight everybody 



210 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

and doesn't quite know what's happening. He was in a bar and had had a 
few too many. He started a fight with the guy standing next to him and there 
was a free-for-all, and in this free-for-all he hit the guy over the head with a 
bottle and killed him. He's being led away by the police here and he's still 
too drunk to know what happened. When he wakes up in the morning and 
finds himself facing a murder rap he's going to be stunned. 

Or he may assume complete responsibility for his behavior. 

As far as the present is concerned this woman has had quite a career. She 
was exposed to lots of money and she stole some I can't think of the term 
they use for stealing by forging numbers in accounts, but anyway she did it. 
She needed money for something very important. But after she had taken it 
she realized what a wrong thing she had done, and somehow the thing she 
had taken the money for didn't seem so important she went and confessed. 
She couldn't give the money back because she'd spent it, so she went to jail. 
But she never did anything wrong again. 

E. THE DURATION OF ANTISOCIAL BEHAVIOR 

Antisocial behavior may represent an isolated incident or the way of life 
of a hero. Further one may ask how enduring are the consequences of the 
behavior, whether isolated or repeated? In the following story the act repre- 
sents no more than an isolated incident, although its consequences are never 
forgotten. 

This picture reminds me of the hero in Jane 'Eyre, He's a man who at some 
time in his life has done what he considers a great wrong but which the 
world for the most part would excuse him for. His deep faith has given him 
a burning desire to reconcile himself in the eyes of his God. He considers him- 
self shackled to this need for reconciliation and is at the point o this picture 
wondering what great thing he can offer God. He toys with the idea of giving 
his life, but considers this sacrilegious in the sense that he's taking the easy 
way out He will go on leading an unhappy life and die at some great 
catastrophe. 

Or antisocial behavior may represent the hero's way of life, with enduring 
consequences. 

Mr. X was a criminal who had committed many crimes, including murder for 
which he was sentenced to pay for with his life. His attorney an old gray 
haired man did not believe that he was alone in this, for it was his firm belief 
that there had been a gang of criminals involved in these crimes. Mr. X did 
not admit that he was alone but he would not tell the names of the other 
people involved no matter how much his attorney pleaded with him. Mr. X 
was a man with a moral code, even if it was the code of criminals which was 
lie only code he had ever known and rather than give up his code he went 
to the chair without revealing the names of the other men. 



DIAGNOSIS OF PERSONALITY: SOCIAL RELATIONSHIPS 211 

F. SEQUELAE OF ANTISOCIAL BEHAVIOR 
What happens to the hero as the result of his antisocial behavior? 

I. PUNISHMENT 

The hero may suffer punishment for his antisocial behavior. 

a) Punishment jrom Exogenous Sources 

Punishment may come from exogenous sources. The hero may be 
punished by the representative of legally constituted authority, as in the 
following story. 

Here a man is being apprehended by a policeman. This man has probably 
been wanted for murder for some time and had been in a state of worry or fear 
in his attempts to lose them. Now that he has been caught it is with an 
expression of relief and his reluctance to struggle, for he is a large man and 
possibly could put up a great struggle, make it appear that he is gkd the 
chase is over. 

Or he may be punished by his victim. 

This is a fellow a young guy, 27, 28, got started in life in wrong way, 
dishonest in business, selfish jaw shows this. Someone he hit badly came in 
and murdered him. This is the scene of the murder. This is the arm of die 
other person involved in it. The fellow might have been strangled. 

Or by another criminal. 

Ah, this, ah, fellow and his friend, they grew up on the other side of the 
tracks and ah they became back to crime again they started of course during 
their, oh, petty thievery and ah before they got very old they were committing 
more major crimes. It happened on a robbery in which they had taken about, 
this fellow, the other fellow had done the hard work in it. He had taken 
the money while this fellow stood guard. The other fellow didn't want to split 
fifty-fifty on it for some reason. This fellow went into a rage and pulled out 
a gun and shot his friend. This is the moment just after he shot him. He 
realized that he was wrong. He has fallen on the floor* He is weeping. He 
actually loved his friend and, and he has, the fellow has, he has just dragged him 
out the door and now he is just sitting and he doesn't know what to do. This is 
a case, it's after this while he is crying the police come in. Somebody has 
reported the shot, and they do come in and the fellow gives himself up. He 
admits everything and the murder and the crimes that have been committed. 
He is given a twenty-five year sentence in the penitentiary. Its point is that 
crime doesn't pay. 

Or by some natural force as in the following story where the hero escapes 
only to be drowned. 

The impression is that this man is up to no good. He looks like a criminal 
type narrow eyes. I've got it now. He stole a lot of money, make it 



212 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

Sioo,ooo and was skipping to another country where he would live in luxury 
and no one would know his background. The ship was sailing along and he 
was very satisfied with himself and relaxing now that he has escaped. But a 
storm blew up which wrecked the ship. All hands were lost so he didn't 
escape his fate after all. 

Or the punishment may come from a supernatural force. 

This shriveled old man was once a pastor in a small town in New England, a 
God-fearing righteous man. But something turned him from the path of 
God. People in the village say he sold his soul to the devil. Whatever it was 
he turned away from God and people and was seen abroad in the company of 
the ne'er-do-wells of the village and often walked by himself in the cemetery 
in the dead of night, and you have to be on friendly terms with the evil spirits 
to do that. Then the scandal broke that rocked the village. The pastor turned 
out to be no more than a common, ordinary thief. He had been stealing from 
neighboring villages for years and burying the money in the cemetery. The 
indignant villagers gathered together and marched to find him. He heard 
that they were coming and hurried to the cemetery hoping to dig up the money 
and escape. As they marched, the people heard ominous sounds in the sky, 
great black clouds gathered and when they reached the gates of the cemetery, 
they saw a sight that none of them will ever forget The pastor stood in the 
midst of the tombstones, busily digging, when there was a terrible thundering, 
the heavens split and a great tongue of lightning reached down and struck 
him where he stood. Many said that God has wanted to punish him and had 
chosen this way. There may have been a few who doubted but no one 
raised his voice. 

i. Nature and duration of exogenous punishment. What is the nature 
and duration of the punishment which follows antisocial behavior? ' The 
punishment may be death, as in the following story where the hero is electro- 
cuted for his crime. 

Dave had been brought up in the slums of one of our big cities and received 
his education from his own imagination. He thought he was wronged in 
being born in poverty and always bore this grudge against the world. Instead 
of making an honest success of himself, he strove to get what he wanted and 
what other people had through crime. He became brutal and hard. 'Til one 
day, upon escaping in a stolen car, he chose to run down an elderly woman 
rather than risk smashing it up. She was an old scrubwoman, mother of seven 
children, whom she slaved to bring up right and turned out to be an old 
neighbor of his. When later caught and faced in the morgue by the mangled 
body, he could not bear it and screeched for mercy before collapsing. He 
later died, coward as he was, in the electric chair, still with distorted mind. 
But we were supposed to regard our lives as luxuries rather than sacrifice. 

Or, it may be life imprisonment. 

These two girls were put into a reform school for committing a crime at a 
very young age. They never had much to do except sit around in a cell, 



DIAGNOSIS OF PERSONALITY: SOCIAL RELATIONSHIPS 213 

and look at the scenery outside. They were never allowed to go out, but 
remained in their cell until death. 

Or, imprisonment for a specific number of years. 

The boy has just told his mother about his committing a robbery. Before they 
were very poor and he decided to steal to give his mother, luxuries in her 
last years. He robbed a bank and told his mother about it She is unhappy 
but tells her son to give himself up the son does this and spends the next ten 
years in jail, 

Or the hero may be imprisoned but released for good conduct before his 
term has been served. 

This man has just committed a terrible crime and his conscience is bothering 
him so that he thinks that people are looking at him and that he is being 
seized by policemen who know of his wrong doings. In desperation (for he 
feels as if people are running after him all the time) he goes to the police and 
confesses his crime, and is put in jail and sentenced to life imprisonment 
Later, however, he is let out for his conduct is like that of any upright citizen. 
He has learned his lesson from his conscience. 

2. Attitude of the hero toward exogenous punishment. What is the hero's 
attitude toward such punishment? 

a) Resistance. The hero may resist apprehension by the law. 

This one has to be dramatic. That's a weird looking house or whatever it is. 
It's in the dead of winter 'and this house is completely covered with snow, 
Some crooks are hiding out in it For months the police have been trying to 
catch up with them, but they're hidden in the country and you can hardly tell 
the house from the snow except for the chimney which sticks up like a 
periscope which in fact is what it is, so that the crooks can see anyone who 
comes up the road. One of the crooks named Tony is posted at the periscope 
and he sees cars moving along the road, he yells to the other men, he's the 
head of them and tells them to stick their guns out through the windows in 
case there is any trouble. The police surround the house thinking that they 
can't be seen, not knowing that the crooks have guns trained on them and are 
watching their every move. The police yell to them to surrender but they just 
answer by firing the guns. Meanwhile they are arranging to escape by means 
of an underground passage. While the police are too busy firing at the house 
to notice, they sneak out one by one and are far away by the time the police 
notice that they are the only ones firing. They keep doing this kind of thing 
and are never caught- 

Or give himself up. 

This man has just committed a terrible crime and his conscience is bothering 
him so that he thinks that people are looking at him and that he is being seized 
by policemen who know of his wrongdoing. In desperation (for he feels a* 
if people are running after him all the time) he goes to the police and confesses 



214 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

his crime and is put in jail and sentenced to life imprisonment Later, how- 
ever, he is let out for his conduct is like that of any upright citizen. He has 
learned his lesson from his conscience. 

But though his wish may be to avoid punishment he may yet give 
himself up. 

This man has just murdered a woman. He was overcome with remorse and 
stunned. ID a little while his mind snaps into action he must cover this up 
some way, no, he must not cover up. He better confess the whole business. 
No, a terrible thing. His mind becomes more and more alert. He began to 
think of a means of getting away without arousing suspicion. He turned her 
over, covered her up and softly left the room. As he went down the stair 
stealthily, he thought he heard a door open on the second floor. He pressed 
against the wall but the noise stopped. He got down the stairs. He walked 
out on the street, looked back once or twice and walked quickly away. He 
had better get out of town. He walked for a number of blocks. His mind 
became hazy; he didn't know what to do. Gleaming lights of a Police Station 
were blinking a few blpcks away. They seemed to be beckoning to him. Yes 
no they were calling to him he walked deliberately up to them walked up 
to the desk and gave himself up. He was sentenced to 25 years for manslaughter. 

b) Attitude toward apprehension. How does the hero regard the fact of 
his capture? He may have lived in a state of anxiety and fear but be glad 
when he is caught that the chase is over. 

Here a man is being apprehended by a policeman. This man has probably 
been wanted for murder for some time and had been in a state of worry or 
fear in his attempts to lose them. Now that he has been caught it is with an 
expression of relief and his reluctance to struggle, for he is a large man and 
possibly could put up a great struggle, make it appear that he is gkd the chase 
is over. 

Often the hero's attitude toward apprehension is implicit. But in some 
instances the hero may describe his feelings of hostility. 

This man held someone up with a gun on a dark street The man put up a 
struggle and they were fighting when a cop heard them and came up and 
asked what was the matter. The man who was doing the robbing got in a 
fight with the cop who tried to arrest him and it wasn't 'til some other cops 
came because they heard the noise that they were able to drag this man off to 
jail. He cursed and tried to get away the whole time until they got him 
to the jail and when they put him in a cell he was still yelling he wanted to 
get out 

c) Conflict* Is the hero in conflict about whether or not to give himself 
up? The heroine o the following story has no such conflict. 

As far as the present is concerned this woman has had quite a career. She 
was exposed to lots of money and she stole some I can't think of the term 



DIAGNOSIS OF PERSONALITY: SOCIAL RELATIONSHIPS 215 

they use for stealing by forging numbers in accounts, but anyway she did it 
She needed money for something very important. But after she had taken it 
she realized what a wrong thing she had done and somehow the thing she 
had taken the money for didn't seem so important she went and confessed. 
She couldn't give the money back because she'd spent it,, so she went to jail. 
But she never did anything wrong again. 

But the hero may be undecided. 

Here we find a young man who has committed a crime, and has been living 
under cover ever since it occurred. The police are after him, and he ponders 
over the fact, whether he should give himself up or not He goes to a very 
close friend, who always helped him out of his troubles. In this picture we 
find him sitting quietly, but worried listening to the elderly gentlemen, as he 
gives him advice, and is trying to persuade him to give himself up. 

If the hero is undecided he may resolve his conflict by giving himself up. 

All those hands, like the hands of a Hindu statue are symbolic. They are 
dragging the man this way and that He can't decide what to do. Over and 
over in his mind he tries to decide. He has committed some atrocity and he 
can't sleep and he can't eat. He wonders if he should confess, his crime and 
take his punishment, he tries to forget but he can't He loses weight, he can't 
eat At last he is so weak he thinks he's going to die and he drags Tiim.e1f 
to the police and confesses. He goes to prison but he sleeps peacefully for 
the first time since he did it 

If the hero is guilty of antisocial behavior and has been caught, is there a 
conflict about whether he should take his punishment or try to prove his inno- 
cence. The hero of the story that follows has no conflict. He does every- 
thing he can to prove his innocence and escape punishment. 

A guy who's trying to beat a murder rap. Probably has done it before and 
uses all the angles, hires a smart lawyer, fixes an alibi, has dozens of witnesses 
who swear they saw him someplace else at the time of the murder. Probably 
even has his mother come and cry at the trial. Of course there's some smart 
detective who sees through it and breaks his alibi. I've probably been listening 
to the radio too much. 

In the following story the hero has an option between accepting his at- 
torney's offer to try to free him on a plea of insanity, or proving his sanity and 
taking his punishment. JEie decides on the latter and is sentenced to hang. 

A week before this this is a court scene the defendant here has been able to 
get this 'majestic or this handsome, white-haired man to defend him. The 
gray-haired gentlemen is a very prominent lawyer. He offered, he offered to 
take the case of this, this fellow. He is a criminal. He has I guess committed 
some sort of fantastic murder and the gentleman decided that he would try to 
free the fellow, and ah, the fee in the case would be very good and it^the 
money that this fellow is interested in. ... Oh, it's one of those. I was think- 



21 6 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION' TEST 

ing. I thought of a, oh, of a trunk murder. I don't know why that came 
in my mind, but ah, we'll just leave it a trunk murder and all, ah, the lawyer is 
just telling him now that he thinks the case is absolutely hopeless and he is 
going to try, there is one way that he can free him and that is to try and 
prove that he is insane. The fellow is sitting there and he is listening. He 
doesn't know quite what to do. His reaction is kind of nil. He is ah.. . 
He realizes he has been in the wrong. He has committed this horrible crime 
and he decides while the lawyer goes on to prove him insane; he talks himself 
out of it and he proves his sanity and he is convicted o this murder and 
sentenced to hang. 

The heroine of the following story also has a similar conflict whicJh she 
resolves in the opposite way. 

This lovely young woman has come from a murder trial In which she was 
the defendant The man with her is her lawyer, who's a little In love with 
her and who defended her and got her off. Of course she is very young and 
lovely which helped. He is telling her that he never doubted that she was 
innocent for a minute. At which a strange look comes over her face. She 
says, "You're wrong I really did commit that murder. I tried to bring myself 
to confess it but I'm too young to die, I couldn't stand the thought and I 
knew you could get me off if you believed I was innocent" He looks at her 
shocked and disillusioned. He's furious to think that she made him a party 
to her deceit and treachery and turns away from her in rage. He says that 
he'll reopen the case. She looks at him sadly but says, 'You can't do that, no 
one can be tried twice for the same crime." 

3) Success or failure of resistance to exogenous punishment. Is the lero 
ahle to resist apprehension successfully? The hero may never be caught as 
in the following story: 

The hands belong to a sort of Robin Hood character. He steals from the rich 
to give to the poor. He and one of his gang are sticking up thus Vealthy 
guy. They do a lot of good and are never caught. 

Or he may wish to avoid apprehension but be unsuccessful in his attempt. 

A second story man. He broke into a house and was using this rope to> get 
away when someone spotted him and called the police. He's hanging on die 
rope in midair, and goes up the rope and tries to make his getaway o-ver the 
roofs of the houses. The police chased him and also surrounded the buildings 
so if he tries to get down he'll walk right into their cordon. The chase goes 
on for a couple of tours but they finally get him. 

If the hero has been imprisoned and attempts to escape, is this attempt 
successful? The hero may be captured in his attempt to escape: 

This a jail break, he's using this rope to lower himself over the prison wall. 
The sirens are screeching and the cops are shooting at tim. He's just about 



DIAGNOSIS OF PERSONALITY: SOCIAL RELATIONSHIPS 217 

to drop to the ground when a bullet gets him in the shoulder and he falls. The 
cops rush down and capture him. His leg is broken and he's shot up but they 
take him to the prison hospital and he gets well. 

Or there may be some uncertainty about the success o his attempt: 

A prisoner is shown escaping by means o a rope from a prison. He is a 
husky fellow and might succeed. If he does he will be very careful for a while 
but soon will get more and more careless. Probably he will be retaken. Accord- 
ing to him it is worth the risk for a little freedom at the least Right now 
though, he is thinking of nothing but his escape and is on sharp lookout for a 
place to run when he gets down. 

Or the hero may escape from prison, never be retaken and live to enjoy 
his ill-gotten gains. 

I see a pirate ship in the Caribbean, flying a Jolly Roger, a very colorful ship 
manned by swarthy sailors wearing gold earrings with daggers through their 
belts. The captain of the sturdy ship is Spanish John a very bloody pirate whose 
reputation has followed him around the world. He takes particular delight hi 
plundering the ships of the British who are his old enemies and have been 
trying to capture him for ten years. It's been a merry chase, but they caught 
up with him in a little harbor and blocked the mouth of the harbor, he was 
completely outnumbered and had to surrender. The British were jubilant and 
put him in irons, but not for long. He made a daring escape and sailed away 
to the island where he has headquarters and lives the rest of his days in 
luxury, leaving only to plunder an occasional ship. 

But this type of story is rare and it is noteworthy that it is placed in a 
remote and romantic setting. 

4) Relatedness of crime and punishment. The hero may or may not be 
explicitly aware of the connection between crime and punishment. There are 
stories in which this connection is not made. The hero may, for example, 
commit an antisocial act for which he is not punished but then suffer an acci- 
dent which is not connected either by the hero or the storyteller to the crime for 
which it might have been a punishment. This may be taken to mean that the 
individual is not altogether aware of the connection between his own anti- 
social impulses and the punishment which he expects. Ordinarily this connec- 
tion is explicitly made in crime and punishment stories, indicating that for most 
individuals the sequence of crime and punishment is something so dramatic 
and painful that awareness of this connection is inevitable. That it is possible 
for an individual to be relatively unaware of this connection is illustrated in 
the following story in which death due to an accident follows antisocial behavior 
with no statement of any explicit connection between the two. 

This is a man who had a great difficulty in establishing himself as a youth in 
his own mind. So he turned to crime as a means to power and glory. One 



21 8 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

foggy night he was finishing up a robbery and the man woke up, he had to 
kill him. He walked out of the building and out into the street not looking 
where he was going and a car ran him down and killed him. 

b) Punishment from Endogenous Sources 

Punishment may issue from within the hero rather than from exogenous 
sources. He may suffer guilt which prevents him from leading a normal life. 
In the following story the servant, although not directly responsible for his 
master's death, suffers guilt which makes him withdraw from the society of 
others. 

A servant has wronged his master. As an indirect cause of this wrong the 
master has died. The master was quite poor and the servant liked his master 
he wasn't mercenary. Now the servant has gone to the grave of his master 
seeking in some way to get relief from his feeling of guilt. He is truly 
penitential. This feeling of guilt will continue to bother him and he will tend 
to be around people less than before. 

Or he may be tormented by thoughts of his deed and commit suicide. 

This man has committed a murder. Some force greater than that of other 
men is preventing him from leading a normal life. He is tormented by the 
thoughts of his deed and is, at the point of this picture, attempting to walk 
off his state of mind. He feels very tired, has a tremendous pressing in his 
head. He doesn't know which way to turn or where to go. He carries on 
like this quite a long while and then takes his own life by drowning himself. 

c) Punishment from Both Endogenous and Exogenous Sources 
Punishment may stem from sources not entirely exogenous nor purely 
endogenous. In the following story the hero is tormented by his conscience 
and gives himself up to exogenous punishment. 

This man had just committed a terrible crime and his conscience is bothering 
him so that he thinks that people are looking at him and that he is being 
seized by policemen who know of his wrongdoing. In desperation (for he 
feels as if people are running after him all the time) he goes to the police and 
confesses his crime and is put in jail and sentenced to life imprisonment 
Later, however, he is let out for his conduct is like that of any upright citizen. 
He has learned his lesson from his conscience. 

Or he may place himself in a position where he knows he will be pun- 
ished by natural forces. The hero of the following story goes to a cave of vam- 
pire bats knowing that they will destroy him. 

Having just committed some crime, this South American has gone to a cave 
of vampire bats, intent on leaving the world in their beaks. He knows that 
they^are instruments of the lower regions, that he won't have to sit very long 
for judgment in their hands before descending to Hell, whence they will 
quickly guide him. 



DIAGNOSIS OF PERSONALITY: SOCIAL RELATIONSHIPS 219 

Or exogenous punishment may lead the individual to inflict further punish- 
ment on himself. 

Because of having done something fiendish this man is in prison. He spends 
all his time staring out the window at the people doing everyday things. His 
sentence is life and he knows that he'll never leave these walls again, and 
tears roll down his checks. The next time a friend comes to see him he asks 
him to slip him a knife which he does and he kills himself, death is better to 
him than a life in prison. 

2. REFORMATION 

The sequel of antisocial behavior may involve reformation by the hero, 
rather than punishment. 

a) Sources 

How is reformation incited? In the following story the reformation of 
the hero is effected primarily through external agencies. Religion, love, or 
friendship leads to a realization of the error of his ways and to his reformation. 

This might have been the case of a man who lived a life of crime and evilness, 
and obscenity, and did not realize his evil ways, till one day through religious 
experience or the finding of a pure love or perhaps the making of a real and 
true friend he sees the real light. Crossing the threshold and leaving behind 
forever this life of unhappiness not unhappiness but evilness. He is constantly 
working away from it in a reactionary way, and thus one day becomes the 
epitome of success and decency in his own community. 

In the next story, punishment is the exogenous agency inciting reformation. 

This man might be a criminal and he's finally cornered and thrown in jail 
with all these bats and owls. Seems to be resigned to his fate, and spends his 
time in jail writing. There's writing paper and some kind of pen down 
there. When he started on his life of crime he never expected to land in jail 
and it has a big effect on him, the horrible monotony and lack of company 
almost drives him out of his mind. He finally gets out and is so glad, he 
resolves never to risk having to go through that again and he gives up crime 
forever. 

Reform may stem from the confluence of an external agency, and a spirit 
which had never been wholly dedicated to evil. In the following story the 
mother is the immediate inciter to reform. She persuades him to return the 
money, but the hero had always been a "good hearted fellow." 

This story starts, this fellow is his mother's pride and joy from child, from 
child, from the time he is born. He is an only child and of course has become 
very spoiled through these years. He grows into a reckless young man, too reck- 
less. He becomes a sort of, oh, he gets involved in, in small crimes and ah, 
yes, oh, particularly robberies and so forth. He is a good hearted fellow but 



220 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

he just gets involved and his mother is aware of this and it, it hurts her very 
much but ah ? she is one of those mothers that, she can't reproach him for 
anything and of course he is always very honest with her and he conies to tell 
her all of his problems when he does get into these scrapes. Up to this 
time, however, he has gotten involved in a major robbery and in making their 
escape they have gone around a corner and some old gentleman has been 
hit by the car that he is driving and he wasn*t driving fast but he was trying 
to get away in a hurry and he hit this old man and because he had taken, 
he had stolen enough money from this concern so that it was actually a major 
robbery but that isn't the thing that bothers him. He is afraid that the old 
man was hit hard enough so that he was killed. He didn't stop so that he 
naturally is a hit-and-run driver too to add to all this trouble. He goes home 
and tells his mother of all this mess. He of course is being hunted by the 
police now. Ah, the mother of course is very worried. She doesn't know 
what to say. She has turned away. She is angry. She realizes that she has 
made mistakes in raising of her son. She gets the boy, she has talked him 
into returning the money that he has stolen and they, she is, she is a religious 
woman and, and she starts praying for this old gendeman. Well, the fellow 
returns the money and gives himself up to the police. He is a kind of a, it's 
a kind of a change of heart; like I said he was a good fellow at heart and so 
the mother in her prayers decides that she will go and see the old gendeman 
that the fellow had hit on the street and very fortunately the gentleman, the 
gentleman is recovering and when the mother and this old fellow get to 
talking they realize that, that they are childhood playmates. They haven't 
seen each other for several years now but ah, when they, she tells the old 
gendeman his story he is about to, well he withdraws his charges against the 
young man and the fellow for having returned the money and these charges 
against him were withdrawn by his, this old gendeman and he is released and 
from then on the outcome is that he lives a very straightforward and happy life. 

Or the incitcr of reformation may be completely endogenous. 

It seems to be a symbolic thing, this man has difficult mental and psychological 
problems. He might have gotten in with a bunch of gamblers and been 
unable to pay his gambling debts and thus drifted into a life of crime doing 
what the gamblers wanted him to. In time he got the money to pay the 
debts but by then he was in so deep he just drifted along. But now it has 
suddenly hit him in the face what's happened to him, how low he has sunk, 
robbing and even killing one man. He's afraid to get out of it, but he is so 
filled with loathing at himself that he can't live with himself if he can't live a 
decent life. No matter what happens he has to be able to live with himself 
or die. He goes away to another part of the country with the hope of starting 
a new life, 

b) Duration of Reformation 

How enduring is the reformation? The hero's character may never again 
suffer moral relapse. 

Awful lot of hands, poor man. He wouldn't be seized by so many hands unless 
ifs in his imagination. He's probably murdered a lot of people and sees these 



DIAGNOSIS OF PERSONALITY: SOCIAL RELATIONSHIPS 221 

hands coming to take him to prison. The fright is so terrible that he mends 
his ways and leads a life of perfect virtue from that time on. The hands 
never come back. 

Or the reformation may be relatively short lived. 

Joe had always been the tough guy of the neighborhood, but when he grew 
older, he used to get into serious difficulties with the law. His father had 
warned him that it was easier to go straight than take the risks he did with 
his mob of petty gangsters, but Joe liked the profit his illegal doings brought 
him. One day he and his pals were caught robbing a warehouse, and they 
each got five years in jail. Joe came out resolved to go straight, but he needed 
a start, so he went to his father who could set him up in his business. Joe's 
father refused to have anything to do with him and turned him out of his 
house. Joe makes several other attempts to find honest work, but no one wants 
to hire a man with a jail sentence. He finally goes in with another gang, and 
is shot during a bank hold-up. 

3. ATONEMENT 

If the crime represents an incident rather than a way of life, the hero may 
atone for his act rather than reform. 

From this picture I gather the impression the young man is in a mental state 
of anxiety and befuddlement because he has shot his best friend while in an 
argument. The life of his friend whom he shot is in doubt and only this 
possible operation can save him. The young man is indeed sorry for his action 
and is waiting and praying in his befuddled state that the operation may prove 
a success so he may atone for his act The operation does prove a success and 
the boy does atone for his act and a lasting friendship develops. 

4. AMORAL SEQUELAE 

Although the individual may suffer or escape some form of punishment 
or reform or atone for his antisocial behavior, the hero may commit anti- 
social behavior in a completely amoral fashion with no thought of punishment 
or guilt. Such a hero is presented in the following story. 

This is Jack the Ripper. It's in London and he has with him the heads of the 
bodies inside the bag he's carrying. He's had a very interesting evening! 
Killed a few people here and there, lots of fun. He*s going back to his room 
and nail the bodies up on the wall or something. I'm not quite sure what he 
does with them. 

II, INTERPRETATION OF ANTISOCIAL BEHAVIOR 

Interpretation of the variables illustrated in Part I follows the general 
method we have employed in the region of love, sex, and marital relation- 
ships. Wherever a single dimension is constant throughout all the stories 



222 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

there is presumptive evidence that this invariance is a reflection of a stable 
element within the personality of the storyteller. We must first determine 
the extent to which antisocial behavior is attributed to heroes. Not infrequently, 
the entire sphere o antisocial behavior is conspicuous by its absence from a 
protocol. The heroes' values and problems may be of an entirely different 
kind. Dedication to the service of humanity, an all consuming interest in 
achievement or the counteraction of some inferiority, a desperate sense of 
loneliness and an attempt to achieve communion with a love object these 
and other values may polarize the main energies of the individual. In the 
pursuit of these ends, antisocial wishes may be of slight import. Conscience is 
the representative of but one member of the family of man's values the 
moral value. Various other values of equal import may be represented in the 
democratic organization of the individual's motivational systems. There are, 
of course, those for whom the moral value is central in a hierarchical organi- 
zation which subordinates all other values to the moral imperative. No less 
frequently however, the hierarchical organization may subordinate the moral 
value and it's representative the super-ego to some other central value, such 
as achievement or the counteraction of inferiority. The following two stories 
told by different individuals illuminate the basic differences between an indi- 
vidual who is concerned with antisocial wishes and the moral problem of their 
control, portrayed in the classic image of the taming of wild horses and the 
individual whose allegory concerns the dangers inherent in the path of life and 
the knowledge requisite for competence in coping with such dangers. For 
both individuals control is basic, but one needs to control his own antisocial 
impulses and the other external forces which are dangerous and which he 
must learn to control and use to his own advantage. 

Hm, we got a, here we have a bunch of cowboys out on the ranch. They, they 
are also horse thieves and at the present time they have just had a heavy day 
drawing up, they have left the farm and they have come out to the, to a, oh, a 
little ah, just kind of hole in the ground where they draw up their plans for 
a big raid on a ranch tonight where they expect to get a lot of horses, men 
with which to start a ranch of their own. I think there are, oh, there must be 
six or seven of them lying around. We only have four in this picture, and, ah, 
anyway ah, they have just made up the plans and now they just kind of, they 
think life is so easy and they have lain down to rest up for the night and, ah, 
there happens to be one double-crosser here. It*s the guy that's looking directly 
at us in the picture, and he is making plans, they have drawn up all the plans 
and he figures well that if, the plans they have doesn't take seven men to do 
it so ah, ah, he decides that he will do it all by himself. He doesn't know 
what he's going to do about these guys but he figures that if he works pretty 
well and can slip away he will be abfe-to finish things off and finish them too. 
He's kind of a heartless guy and a few minutes after this picture is taken he, 
he tries to slip away and another guy wakes up and decides to follow him and 
when they have got over to this ranch and are ready to open the gates and 



DIAGNOSIS OF PERSONALITY: SOCIAL RELATIONSHIPS ^23 

start the horses stampeding out, running away. It, it can be done on account 
of this corral is, is several miles from the house of the ranch owner and this 
fellow, the one fellow has disposed of the two guards that have been watching 
the corral tonight and so all he has to do now is, is stam, get the horses out and 
start them away, running away on a stampede. Well, just as he is opening 
the corral this, the fellow that's behind him in this picture comes up to him 
and wonders what he has been doing, double-crossing and all that and the 
fellow who has started double-crossing the first time decides well, maybe the 
two of them might just as well do this little job together and share the profits 
that they can get and start their own ranch and so being both ruthless fellows 
they make this agreement and they don't give a hang about the other guys. 
Well, they open the corral and chase out the hundred and fifty head of there, 
the, these horses out of the ah, ah, corral and ah, it's kind of funny. They 
find out that the, the horses that have been in this corral are wilder than they 
thought. They are just, they don't know why, they are wild horses but they 
are wild horses, but anyway, they actually start on a stampede. They are unable 
to control them and so they start running down the valley and they are headed 
for the spot where these fellows have been asleep. There are still five fellows 
lying on the ground, and, ah, and, ah, and, ah, they are not aware that these, 
this stampede of horses are headed in their direction. I don't know why these, 
these two fellows should get all excited and kind-hearted all at once but they 
decide they had better warn the guys that the horses are coming; so they try 
and dash around and, ah, and, ah, head the fellows off but they are not making 
much better time than the stampeding horses but as they get down there they 
yell at the guys and they jump off their horses, shake 'em, and get them all 
up and just at that moment when they are climbing on their horses the 
stampede comes dashing into them and the guys are knocked on the ground 
and the whole, this whole, all seven of the guys are just trampled to death 
and of course whatever happens to the horses, they are set out on this wild 
plain again and will have to be, ah, ah, caught if they are ever going to be 
tame. 

I walked into this apparent hole in the ground and looking with a good deal 
of wonder when a man walked up to me and said, 'Take this and any time 
you need any help why merely ask it and take your tool and use it whenever 
danger approaches." I just walked along this. . . . Well, it was some sort of 
a shiny metal instrument and moon-shaped with various embossings on it 
almost like a shield a small one. And I walked along througji, through what 
seemed to be the path of life passing these various dangers and for each one 
assuming different exteriors according to my need and protection, sometimes 
shell of an armadillo, at other times the spines of a porcupine and the scales 
of a fish and all sorts of horrible obstacles, sometimes webbed feet sometimes 
to cross water, other times feet equipped for climbing over huge stones. Finally 
the path went around, turned around and led to a, came to a fork in the road 
one side seemed to point outward toward earth and continuation of life and 
the other one toward more wanderings and more knowledge, more experience 
in the mysteries that I had been through. I don't know which one I took, 
though. Do I have to decide that? [Laughs], . . . Well, I took the one out 
to the earth and thought perhaps I could make use of some of the allegory and 
knowledge that I had picked up in my trip. 



224 ^^ THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

If the protocol does reveal antisocial behavior, the variability of the dimen- 
sions illustrated in Part I must be examined and explained. If in one story 
the hero tries to escape but is ultimately punished for his crimes by imprison- 
ment we could not interpret this story until we had compared this with other 
stories concerned with immoral behavior. If all other stories revealed a hero 
who expressed antisocial behavior only to suffer imprisonment after an unsuc- 
cessful attempt to escape this punishment, we would assume that the indi- 
vidual feared external punishment for any expression of his antisocial wishes 
but had not introjected moral norms sufficiently to experience remorse or guilt. 
But if we found other stories in which the hero suffered acute remorse rather 
than punishment for his misbehavior we would have to explain this difference. 
We have found that the same individual may experience guilt for one kind of 
sin and fear punishment for some other sin he will, for example, feel guilt 
for sexual misbehavior but fear punishment for antisocial aggression. Or 
the hero may, in two stories, experience guilt or fear external punishment for 
the same kind of sin but the difference may depend on the violence of the 
crime. Thus a hero who injures a person may fear the law, whereas a hero 
who kills someone experiences intense guilt. Or it may depend on the hero's 
role in the crime. There will commonly be found heroes who fear punish- 
ment for crimes committed with the cooperation of allies, but who feel guilt for 
crimes committed without the cooperation of allies. Again, guilt may be the 
sequel to crimes instigated by the hero, fear of punishment the result of crimes 
another person has persuaded the hero to commit. 

Stories vary not only in the interaality or externality of punishment but in 
the heroes' attitude toward the punishment and in the severity and duration 
of punishment. There are important variations hi the type of antisocial be- 
havior expressed, the targets of such behavior and the part played by the 
hero in crime. All these variations between stories in a single protocol can 
usually be explained either as the effect of some important cause which varies 
in these stories, or as a cause of different effects which result from variations in 
any of these dimensions. 

Any story of antisocial behavior must also be analyzed with due regard to 
the possibility of the expression of repressed wishes. Thus the objects toward 
which antisocial behavior is directed may express varying degrees of remote- 
ness. The role of the hero in antisocial behavior may indicate those conditions 
which are sufficiently remote and safe to permit the expression of repressed 
wishes; for example, when someone else persuades the hero to commit a 
crime tie individual may express a wish which he could not express if he felt 
that he was directly responsible. The social context may provide similar 
remoteness and safety. As we saw in the case of Z, the assistance of allies 



DIAGNOSIS OF PERSONALITY: SOCIAL RELATIONSHIPS 225 

permitted Z's heroes to aggress upon father surrogates. The story of the 
slaves who murdered their master is an example of this. 

One must also consider the temporal characteristics of antisocial behavior. 
Whether antisocial wishes have their origin in childhood, or in later life is a 
matter of import. In order to determine this we must compare stories con- 
cerned with antisocial behavior in which the hero is a child, an adolescent, 
and an adult. We must then examine the extent to which the hero's wishes 
have changed from childhood to adulthood. Thus, there are protocols that 
delineate a childhood turbulent with moral conflict which continues unre- 
solved into adulthood. In other"protocols adulthood is portrayed as relatively 
free of the moral conflicts of childhood. Analysis of the age of the heroes who 
are concerned with these problems may indicate that these conflicts were re- 
solved in late childhood, adolescence, or in early adulthood. There are also 
protocols in which childhood is marked by freedom from moral conflict but 
adolescence is particularly stormy, with serious sequelae in adulthood. Finally 
there are protocols which indicate that the individual suffered acute moral 
problems in early childhood, solved these problems in late childhood but suf- 
fered reactivation of the same conflicts in adolescence or adulthood. In the 
analysis of such sequences one may use the technique employed in the in- 
vestigation of changes in parent-child relationships. Stories of antisocial be- 
havior should be placed in chronological order, according to the age of the 
hero in each story. Changes which may be attributed to differences in the 
age of the hero should then be analyzed to illuminate exacerbations and 
resolutions of moral conflicts as these fluctuate in time. 

We may also examine the impact of antisocial behavior on other regions. 
It is possible through such an examination to illuminate the importance of other 
regions for the individual whose heroes express antisocial behavior. If the hero 
who commits a crime is concerned with the effect this will have on his family, 
this indicates the importance o the latter to the hero and to the storyteller. 
When an individual tells a story of crime he has the option of describing the 
consequences of his behavior either in terms of his own fear or remorse or in 
terms of the effect which this will have on his family or love object or on his 
work or career. In the following story, for example, the consequences of 
antisocial behavior for the hero's marital relationship are such that the hero 
ultimately reforms. 

There was once a young scientist in the late nineteenth century who became 
a genius in the use of explosive chemicals. He loved a young girl very dearly 
and married her. They were married a great many years, during which time 
he received great amounts of money from various countries. But the various 
explosives he invented were for uses of destruction. He gradually acquired 
a great desire, or lust, for w money and power and he continued to invent these 
deadly explosives, each one more powerful and deadly than the other. But 



226 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

beneath this lust was imbedded his IOVQ for this girl for whom he thought he 
was doing best by showering on her these fabulous riches. Day after day, she 
pleaded with him to give up this business and devote his time to medical 
science instead. But his lust grew until, one day, after having made a terrifically 
explosive powder, he left it in his laboratory while he went off to negotiate for 
its sale. While he was gone, for some unknown reason which we can attribute 
to nature, this powder exploded, blowing up the house and killing his wife. 
Though still a young man, the loss of his wife suddenly made him realize the 
power of nature and his wrongdoing. The loss of his wife affected him so 
that within the next few years he aged almost twenty years. He gave up his 
malicious lust and turned his ability to medical science. Day after day he 
visited the grave of his wife, until finally he died an old man with great 
accomplishments in the medical field behind him and a full realization of what 
his lust had cost him. 

The parent-child relationship is disturbed by the antisocial behavior of the 
hero in the following story. The mother persuades the son to return the 
money. He has a change o heart and reforms. 

This story starts, this fellow is his mother's pride and joy from child, from child, 
from the time he is born. He is an only child and of course he has become 
very spoiled through these years. He grows into a reckless young man, too 
reckless. He becomes a sort of, oh, he gets involved in, in small crimes and 
ah yes, oh, particularly robberies and so forth. He is a good hearted fellow 
but he just gets involved and his mother is aware of this and it, it hurts her 
very much, but ah, she is one of those mothers that, she can't reproach him for 
anything and of course is always very honest with her and he comes to tell 
her all of his problems when he does get into these scrapes. Up to this time, 
nowever, he has gotten involved in a major robbery and in making their escape 
they have gone around a corner and some old gentleman has been hit by the 
car that he is driving and he wasn't driving fast but he was trying to get 
away in a hurry and he hit this old man and because he had taken, he had 
stolen enough money from this concern so that it was actually a major robbery 
but that isn't the thing that bothers him. He is afraid that the old man was 
hit hard enough so that he was killed. He didn't stop so that he naturally is 
a hit-and-run driver too to add to all this trouble. He goes home and tells 
his mother of all this mess. He of course is being hunted by the police now. 
Ah, the mother of course is very worried. She doesn't know what to say. She 
has turned away. She is angry. She realizes that she has made mistakes in 
the raising of her son* She gets the boy, she has talked him into returning 
the money that he has stolen and they, she is, she is a religious woman and 
she starts praying for this old gentleman. Well, the fellow returns the money 
and gives himself up to the police. He is a kind of a, it's kind of a change of 
heart, like I said he was a good fellow at heart and so the mother in her 
prayers decides that she will go and see the old gentleman, the gentleman is 
recovering and when the mother and this old fellow get to talking they realize 
that they are childhood playmates. They haven't seen each other for several 
years now but ah, when they, when she tells the old gentleman his story he 
is about to, well he withdraws his charges against the young man and the 



DIAGNOSIS OF PERSONAIJTY: SOCIAL RELATIONSHIPS 227 

fellow for having returned the money and these charges against him were 
withdrawn by his, this old gentleman and he is released and from then on the 
outcome is that he lives a very straightforward and happy life. 

Or the hero may, as in the following story, think of his lost career as he 
is going off to jail. 

The hands belong to the law and they are taking this man off to jail. There 
is a long train ride before they reach the penitentiary and this man has plenty 
of time to think over what he's done. Particularly he thinks about his job 
and how he hoped someday to be a great lawyer, but the mess he was in now 
had queered that. Even if it weren't for the jail sentence which would make 
passing the bar impossible he'd be too old when he got out, he'd be lucky if 
he could get any kind of a job. 

Because antisocial behavior in the TAT usually represents suppressed or 
repressed material, it will be found most frequently in the protocols of normal, 
neurotic and psychotic individuals. It will in general be found less promi- 
nent in the protocols of those whose behavior is actually antisocial. Kutash's 
study of psychopathic defective criminals (49) revealed these individuals to 
be much more concerned with separation anxiety, ambition, and guilt than with 
the wish to commit antisocial behavior. Whether these same individuals 
might have described more antisocial behavior in their stories had they been 
tested before they were imprisoned we do not know. But we do know that 
the actual antisocial behavior of the individual telling TAT stories may have 
no 'representation whatever in his stories. It has been the writer's experience 
in "blind" analysis of the TAT's of children and adolescents who presented 
serious behavior problems that he has, in almost every instance, misdiagnosed 
the specific behavior problem which was the concern of the parent and thera- 
pist. Paradoxically, this was not infrequently the virtue of the diagnosis based 
on the TAT protocol. Thus the stories of a 7 year old boy revealed a terrified 
young hero who faced a physically threatening world alone, his parents con- 
spicuous by their absence or death. His heroes were obsessed with the problem 
of time, centering on the time of the parents' return. In response to the violin 
picture, the hero misses the presence of his mother: "And maybe Jiis mother's 
away and he wants to show it to her." In the third story, rather than com- 
plaining of coercive parental dominance the hero laments the absence of his 
parents: "Well this guy might be crying because probably his mother or 
father died and he's all alone.*' When he is with his father the hero is aware 
of physical danger but he believes his father can cope with this. 

And they're going to go out in the boat pretty soon. And the boat won't sink 
and it doesn't have any v leaks in it. And the father will row. Well, it'll end up 
that it doesn't sink. Nobody gets hurt and they'll get back safely. 



228 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

However, the child feels capable of coping with this dangerous world if 
his parents have taught him how to take care of himself. 

I think the guy is in a log cabin. He's wondering and he's seeing something. 
And he wonders inside if he should get his gun. And he doesn't have any 
mother or father. His mother and father are dead, or they're away. He's about 
ten and before his father and mother left they taught him how to work and he 
shoots his own supper and breakfast and gets fish out of a brook. 

Incidental remarks made during the inquiry were the only clue to his 
overt behavior. He talked about how strong he was, how he had cut down 
an oak tree and could pull nails with his hands. He boasted that other boys 
were afraid of him. The discrepancy between the tone of these remarks and 
the characteristics of his heroes made it clear that this was the fagade of an 
anxious child. I was not prepared, however, to learn that he was responsible 
for setting fires. The TAT had been administered in an effort to discover 
the motivation of this behavior. Analysis of the TAT, however, revealed 
neither a preoccupation with fire setting nor evidence of any type of antisocial 
behavior. The physical dangers of a threatening world in the absence of his 
parents, his relative security in the presence of a beloved and powerful father^- 
these were his preoccupations. Setting of fires was instigated by a more 
dominant and more active playmate who persuaded this child to join him. A 
short time after this, his playmate moved away. This frightened and lonely 
child never set another fire, partly because the other child's influence was re- 
moved and partly because his parents were persuaded to give him more atten- 
tion and guidance. The precise meaning of his overt antisocial behavior was 
never determined, nor did the TAT illuminate its meaning, although it pro- 
vided reassurance that the wish to destroy per se did not underlie the subject's 
antisocial behavior. 

Let us consider another "blind" analysis, that of a protocol of a 14 year 
old girl. In this case the only information available to the writer was the 
fact that both mother and father were living, and that she had one younger 
and two older brothers. Analysis of this record revealed a deeply repressed 
hostility towards the brothers and an intense longing* for the exclusive affec- 
tion of the mother. In the following story there is a wish fulfillment phantasy 
indicating what this girl hoped she might mean to her mother. 

This one's not too hard. This woman had a great happiness come into her 
life and she's bursting into the room trying to find her daughter to tell her 
about it. OK dear, I can't think. Her daughter is nowhere to be found. She 
looks every place for her. After giving up hope, she finally hears that her 
daughter was lulled. Murdered. After this the woman has great sorrow and 
never lives a happy life again since she lost her daughter. 



DIAGNOSIS OF PERSONALITY: SOCIAL RELATIONSHIPS 22p 

Contrast the effect of the daughter's death on the mother with the effect 
of the son's death. 

This woman has just been to a party and something terrible has happened 
to her. A child has been drowned out in the ocean. She hears him calling 
and goes running out When she goes out to meet him she finds he's dead. 
This woman up above standing behind the tree is the child's, nurse and she's 
trying to find what's the matter with the child's mother. The mother after 
this is very unhappy about life and she didn't think there was anything to live 
for after her child was gone. She's living in the country, not the city, at the 
time and she moved out in to the country and with her husband and the 
nurse lived very happily and she never had any more children. 

There is also a wish expressed here. She would like the younger child 
to drown so that she might live happily ever after with the mother and 
father. The condition of such happiness would be that the mother have no 
more children. Clearly this girl is tired of playing the role of nurse to the 
younger child. In the story "she's trying to find what's the matter with the 
child's mother." There is no thought that the nurse might be interested in 
the child whose care is her responsibility. This story avoids direct expres- 
sion of her hostility. It is a nurse rather than a daughter. The son drowns 
through no fault of the nurse and the nurse continues to live with the mother 
and father. This accidental death of the boy is not the nurse's expressed wish. 
In the story that follows there is again an indirect expression of the girl's 
hostility towards her brothers. 

This boy is kissing his mother and he's just come home from the service and 
he's so excited that neither of them are laughing or crying, just very sad. The 
boy's twin brother was with him but he was killed in the war. This boy 
was the only one who came home out of three. He starts to tell his mother 
about all the horrible adventures he's had. She's so grief stricken by it that 
she faints. After a few months the mother dies and the boy is left all by 
himself. He gets married. After a few months he gets divorced and is left 
all by himself to be a bachelor. Thaf s a very unsuccessful ending. 

Again, no overt aggression is expressed toward the brother but the 
mother is taken away by death and the brother is eventually 'left all by 
himself to be a bachelor.'* Nor is he permitted the satisfaction of a substi- 
tute mother. Thus far there has been no indication of any hostility toward 
the brother. Our evidence, such as it is, is the result of inference. We have 
found only that she separates mother and son and contrives to unite "nurse'* 
and mother and permits the mother to have no more children. Though she 
had been able to forget the death of her son, the mother is grief-stricken and 
inconsolable at the separation from her daughter. In none of this is there any 
evidence of aggression, except by inference. No one has committed a single 
aggressive act, nor has there been mention of any feeling of hostility. Inasmuch 



230 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

as we suspect some repressed aggression motivating these stories, where shall 
we look for evidence of it? According to our theory of repression we would 
expect some aggression to be expressed under more "remote*' circumstances. 
We have noted before that card n depicting a dragon in a prehistoric setting 
is peculiarly capable of eliciting aggressive phantasies because the animal and 
the setting are sufficiently remote to allow aggression to be displaced. If the 
aggression were directed toward the parent we should expect that one of the 
human figures might be pitted against the dragon in a life and death struggle. 
If an Oedipus complex existed we might expect the struggle to terminate in the 
rescue of the parent figure from the clutches of the dragon. But these common 
displacements do not appear in this case. 

Are these animals? This is an animal that's about to attack the buffalo. And 
this animal is getting ready to jump out of a cavern and he's about ready to 
attack. And the buffalo sets a trap for him. Just about as the animal is about 
to attack the buffalo, his foot slips and over the cliff the buffalo goes. The 
ledge below saves him and the animal that was about to attack the other 
animal hits his head looking down at the buffalo. The animal loses his 
balance and falls over onto the next ledge where the buffalo is. The buffalo 
won his battle. I guess that's all. 

Although the picture provided the possibility of conflict between human 
beings and the dragon, she has elected to disregard this possibility. The 
struggle is made more "remote" than is customary for this card; since it is a 
straggle between two animals rather than a person and an animal. We would 
suppose that this happened because of the very strong inhibition of her ag- 
gression, and the possibility that two animals better symbolize her self and 
her sibling. Further indication of the strength of the repression of her ag- 
gression is seen in the fact that although the "battle" is won by the buffalo, 
the animals never come to grips with each other. "Accidents" happen even 
in this "battle." 

Let us turn then to the card in the female series designed to elicit aggres- 
sion, card 1 8 GF (a woman has her hands squeezed around the throat of 
another woman whom she appears to be pushing backwards across the banister 
of a stairway). We would predict that the somewhat ambiguous figure of 
the woman who is being strangled would be changed to a young boy, but 
not portrayed as her brother and that the older woman would be changed to a 
girl. We would further predict on the basis of the inhibition of aggression 
thus far evidenced, that the story would either be completely unaggressive 
or that if aggression were expressed, it would appear to the heroine as a 
completely "foreign" impulse which might be disclaimed as an abnormality 
or if not disclaimed would produce overwhelming guilt. 

This boy and girl were playing together when the girl became very angry 
with the boy. She's about to strangle him. She's putting her fingers in his 



DIAGNOSIS OF PERSONALITY: SOCIAL RELATIONSHIPS 23! 

car and pushing his eardrums in. The boy screams for help but that does no 
good because the woman has got her fingers in his throat. The woman the 
girl has the boy almost dead when she suddenly realizes what she is doing. She 
stops then and there and leaves the boy and runs out of the house. She's so 
terrified that she goes and commits suicide. The boy lives to be happy ever 
after. 

The aggressive woman is portrayed as a girl, although there is an oscilla- 
tion between these alternatives. The other woman has been turned into a 
"boy." No reason is given for the suddenness of her anger, and there is no 
elaboration of the transition between this feeling and the resultant behavior. 
After she "became very angry" the next moment "she's about to strangle him." 
This she does quickly. She is so intent on this that "the girl has the boy 
almost dead when she suddenly realizes what she is doing." We had pre- 
dicted that the aggression would be something "foreign" and disclaimed by 
the heroine. This was not an altogether mistaken expectation, but the story 
suggests certain qualifications. The fact that the girl could become "very 
angry" while playing with the boy suggests that her aggression is both intense 
and volatile under certain conditions. But the immediate translation of this 
feeling into behavior was permitted only because it was not so intense that 
she wished to kill the boy. It is only in the act of aggression that the girl's 
hostility grows more intense, until finally "the girl has the boy almost dead 
when she suddenly realizes what she is doing." Presumably she would not 
have expressed this aggression had she realized before the full extent of her 
wish, since "she stops then and there and leaves the boy and runs out of the 
house. She's so terrified that she goes and commits suicide.'* In atonement 
for this act she permits this young boy "to be happy ever after." All other 
brothers have been killed, separated from the mother, or left to lead lonely 
lives. We are told then, that if she ever expressed her hostility, its intensity 
would know no limit. But if she were to realize the extent of her wish she 
could not completely express it. The heroine commits suicide because of 
her realization of what she has almost done, and not because she has mur- 
dered the boy. He is permitted to live happily ever after. But her terror at 
the realization of what she has almost done is sufficient to cause her to take 
her life. Her aggression can never be completely expressed and even its 
partial expression would overwhelm her with terror, if she were to realize 
the extent of her aggression. It was for these reasons that the writer doubted 
that the problem presented by this child was one of aggressive behavior. 
But the form it might take could be no more than guessed. The problem 
which concerned the parent and therapist was compulsive stealing. It is 
unclear whether this derives from her wish for her mother's attention or her 
hostility towards her brothers, or both. It would seem more likely that it /is 
an expression of her longing for her mother rather than an expression of hos- 



2J2 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

tility, since the former is to some extent a conscious wish, and to some extent 
repressed in so far as it is connected with killing the siblings. She is 
capable of a wishful phantasy in which she as nurse lives happily with the 
mother but she is incapable of expressing her aggression towards her brothers 
in her stories. We would suppose that the compulsive stealing stems from her 
wish for love, on the basis of our assumption that the more deeply repressed 
the wish the less counterpressure is capable of producing either symptom 
formation, anxiety, or a breaking through of the repressed impulse. 

The wishes of these two individuals, as they are expressed in the TAT, 
and their overt antisocial behavior bear no obvious resemblance to each other. 
But if we were not able to explain the meaning of the antisocial behavior, we 
have at least been given insight into the psychic soil in which this behavior 
took root insight which enabled therapy to attack the causal conditions of 
the symptom despite relative ignorance of their meaning. 

SUMMARY 

In Part I we illustrated dimensions useful in the analysis of antisocial 
behavior. We first examined the form of antisocial behavior and then the 
motivation of such behavior. We saw that these were independent dimen- 
sionsany motive might lead to any type of antisocial behavior. Aggression, 
for example, might be the consequence of jealousy, the wish to be like others, 
or the wish to help someone. A feeling of inferiority might lead to aggression, 
stealing, or antisocial dominance. We then examined the direction of anti- 
social behavior and saw that it might be directed against an individual, some 
particular class or group, or against the social order. The next dimension 
of analysis was the role of the hero in anti-social behavior whether the hero 
or others instigated antisocial behavior. Then we examined the social context 
whether the hero committed his crime alone or with the cooperation of allies, 
and whether the hero regarded himself as a member of an in-group or an 
out-group in his antisocial behavior. We next examined the acceptance of 
responsibility whether the hero assumed responsibility for his behavior or 
felt that he was really innocent or not responsible for his behavior. Examining 
the duration of antisocial behavior, we saw that this might represent an isolated 
incident or the hero's way of life. We then examined the sequelae of antisocial 
behavior- whether the hero suffered punishment and whether punishment 
came from exogenous or endogenous sources. We examined the nature and 
duration of exogenous punishment and the attitude of the hero toward such 
punishment whether he resists punishment or gives himself up and whether 
there is any conflict about the latter. We examined his attitude toward appre- 
hensionwhether the hero was relieved that the chase was over or hostile 
toward his captors. We then examined the success or failure of resistance to 



DIAGNOSIS OF PERSONALITY : SOCIAL RELATIONSHIPS 233 

punishment. We saw that successful resistance to punishment is uncommon 
and usually placed in a remote setting indicative of a repressed wish. We also 
examined the extent to which crime and punishment are connected. Usually 
the punishment is the consequence of a crime, but occasionally one finds stories 
in which this connection is not explicit. This usually signifies that the story- 
teller is not altogether aware of the relation between his antisocial wishes and 
their punishment. Another sequel of antisocial behavior was reformation on 
the part of the hero, either incited through external agencies or the result of 
inner promptings. Such reformation we saw might be permanent or short- 
lived. A third sequel of antisocial behavior was atonement, when the crime 
represented an incident rather than a way of life. Finally we examined 
amoral sequelae in which the hero commits crimes in a completely amoral 
fashion with no thought either of punishment or guilt. These are uncommon 
stories, and the remoteness of either object, time, or setting which is character- 
istic of these stories signifies deeply repressed material. 

In Part II we considered the interpretation of these variables. We said 
first that the general techniques employed in analyzing the dimensions of love, 
sex, and marital relationships could be employed in the region of antisocial 
behavior. We must first examine the extent to which antisocial behavior is 
mentioned in the protocol, since other values may be much more important to 
the individual than moral values. If the latter finds representation we must 
determine whether any dimensions are invariant in the protocol, since these 
represent stable elements in the personality of the storyteller. When the dimen- 
sions vary from story to story it is the task of interpretation to explain sucli 
variation as the cause or effect of concomitant variation of other elements within 
the stories. We said also that attention must be paid to the possibility that 
repressed wishes may be expressed in any story concerning antisocial behavior. 
We next examined the temporal characteristics of antisocial behavior and sug- 
gested that the techniques employed in analyzing changes in the parent-child 
relationship could be applied to changes in antisocial behavior. We also 
examined the impact of antisocial behavior on other regions for the light it 
shed on the importance of these regions for the individual. We then examined 
in some detail the protocols of a child and an adolescent, both of whom 
presented behavior problems. We saw that the TAT shed little light on the 
meaning of the antisocial behavior, which was indeed conspicuous by its 
absence from the protocol; what it did reveal however, enabled the therapist 
to attack the root of the problem despite a relative ignorance of the meaning 
of the symptoms. 



CHAPTER IX 

DIAGNOSIS OF PERSONALITY: WORK AND 
VOCATIONAL SETTING 

Contemporary vocational guidance, not unmindful of the relevance of 
personality diagnosis to an assessment of the individual's abilities and capacities, 
employs the interview and questionnaire methods for this purpose. These, 
however, may be more profitably employed if supplemented by protective 
techniques. 

Assessment of an individual's needs in the work region differs in no 
essential manner from any other variety of personality diagnosis. It is as 
simple or as complex as a determination of the conditions of a neurosis and 
of no less urgent import to the individual. We do not believe that every 
individual who seeks vocational guidance is, or should be, in quest of psycho- 
therapy. In the writer's experience only a small percentage of those seeking 
vocational guidance require psychotherapy. In the majority of cases seen by 
the writer the individual in quest of vocational guidance has accurately diag- 
nosed the region of his life most crucial to his future well-being. He may, 
however, on first appearance in the office of the vocational counselor, be 
anxious and confused and appear to be more in need of therapy than guid- 
ance. Nonetheless, accurate diagnosis of the individual's work needs, his 
acceptance of the diagnosis, and its translation into specific vocational behavior 
may ultimately result in the integration of the individual. 

An accurate assessment of the precise meaning of work to the individual 
is of considerable import in such diagnosis. The irreducible minimum of con- 
ditions necessary if he is to work at all, the conditions which are "ideal" and 
mobilize the individual's best energies, and the conditions which inhibit or 
seriously interfere with his functioning in the work region must all be 
determined. 

We shall turn our attention first to the characteristics of the individual's 
work and the variety of meanings implicit in work. 

I. THE CHARACTERISTICS OF WORK 
A. THE MEANING OF WORK FOR THE INDIVIDUAL 

The goals for which individuals in American society strive and the satis- 
factions they enjoy or hope to enjoy reflect the varieties of motives generated 
by a complex social order. Although all work is to some extent motivated by 
dissatisfaction, it is profitable to distinguish between work oriented primarily 



DIAGNOSIS OF PERSONALITY: WORK SETTING 235 

away from unpleasant or frustrating circumstances and work oriented primarily 
toward some positive goal which excites and mobilizes the individual's energies. 
The former type of work is undertaken by those who are driven by a sense 
of inferiority, by poverty, by unhappiness in love, by the wish to be free of 
dependence on others, or by any anxiety, whatever its source. Such an indi- 
vidual's work is rewarding insofar as it removes him from these frustrations. 
But an individual may also work for more positive goals. The vision of future 
fame or competence or the wish to create something of value of to dedicate 
himself to the welfare of others may excite and inspire the individual and 
sustain his work. For many individuals Jboth types of motivation are found 
together, with varying proportions of the negative and positive components. 
Let us consider first a sample of stories illustrating work which is motivated 
by forces essentially negative in quality. 

I. WORK NEGATIVELY MOTIVATED 

In the following story, work is motivated primarily by the hero's feeling 
of inferiority, which he strives to overcome through hard work. 

Here is a picture of a young man participating in a gym class. This young 
man was probably underdeveloped as a young child and consequently was 
subject for the taunts of his fellow youths for his inability to play or participate 
in the more strenuous games. With a feeling of inferiority he made up his 
mind that he would take every means at his disposal to develop himself. 
Entering into gym classes he worked hard and in time did develop the bulging 
muscles and the stamina that seemed so necessary to win the respect of his 
fellow youths. Here in this picture he is winning the rope climbing event and, 
from the expression on his face, a great deal of satisfaction. 

He may work to overcome poverty and financial insecurity. 

They're very poor and down and out, and this poor guy works like a dog to 
keep his head above water. He's hoping, if he works hard enough, the boss 
will notice him and maybe give him a raise. He's got to get some money to 
pay bills, his wife's been sick and he has to pay the doctor. But it doesn't 
seem to do any good. No matter how hard he works his pay check is the 
same at the end of every week, and it isn't enough. 

Or to overcome rejection by a love object. In the following story a woman, 
rejected in love, finds satisfaction in work. 

This woman is pleading with this man to leave his work for a while and relax. 
She offers herself to him. The man here seems to be hesitating between his 
duty and his love, but actually he knows which really matters to him. Besides, 
he will soon grow tired of the woman and the temporary pleasure is nothing 
compared with the rewards of his work, So he fools around with her for a 
while and then throws her off without remorse because after all she got what 
she wanted and he gave as much as was his to give. The woman takes it 



236 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

badly at first but she recovers and throws herself into her own work with 
greater determination. She is thrilled to find how well she makes out on her 
job. 

Or to overcome the psychological effects of a physical handicap. 

The woman has just had a fit of crying for she is horribly deformed and rushes 
iato her father's arms for comfort. Everyone excludes her from their social 
circles because she is so ugly. Her father tells her that the thing that counts most 
is the real person, not one's physical appearance. She determines that she is 
not going to let this ruin her life. She takes up the study of psychology and 
turns out to be a great authority on the subject And people are so interested 
in what she has to say that they forget what she looks like. 

Or to be free of dependence on others. 

He's a veteran home from the wars, and at first he's happy to be home and 
to see his family again. They're very happy to see him and everyone is very 
nice to him. But after a while he feels that he wants to get out OL. 
his own, he wants to get a job. He can't go on being dependent on hi& 
family forever. He talks to them about it but they can't seem to realize that 
he's grown up in the years he's been away. They tell him to wait awhile 
and then to go to college, and then to think about getting a job. He 
tries to explain that he couldn't take it being dependent on them for four 
more years, that he's grown up in the years he's been in the army, he's not a 
baby any more. He wants to get out and make his own money. They don't 
understand and he feels it's hopeless to try to explain any more. So he packs 
his things and leaves. He's saying goodbye to his mother here. 

Or to overcome his aggression. 

A very impetuous man. He's just 'had a fight with his boss. They're talking 
here and in a minute they'll have a fight, and both will say a lot of things they 
don't mean. The man will come out of the boss's office still mad and will sit 
down to work. Furiously working, calms him, and gradually he will begin 
to feel sorry and will go and apologize to his boss. 

Or to overcome anxiety. 

She looks worried. She's worried and she doesn't quite know what she's 
worried about She's just worried about everything. She tries to get her mind 
off her worries and throws herself into a book. She's studying. The time it's 
time for her to go to bed, she's feeling much better, not worried any more. 

2. WORK POSITIVELY MOTIVATED 

But work may have more positive meaning for the hero. He may work to 
become famous. 

The girl is daydreaming. She wants to become a great artist, the kind that 
people admire. She is a very smart girl and she wants to make a name or 



DIAGNOSIS OF PERSONALITY : WORK SETTING 237 

reputation, for herself. So she studies diligently and develops into a renowned 
person. 

Or to become great. 

This looks like a little boy who wants to become a great violin player. He, I 
mean now he is just dreaming about what a great man he will be. He 
eventually becomes a great violin player and is acclaimed by the world. 

Or to create something of beauty. 

This man is very young. Ever since he was a small child he has been interested 
in music. He has never been able to express his feelings about it but he has 
always wanted to take lessons. His favorite instrument is the violin. He has 
begun to take lessons and he likes it very much. As he first begins to under- 
take his instrument, after he has studied a while he realizes the difficulty of 
what he has undertaken and compares his playing with that of the old masters. 
He realizes how different it is. What he is learning and doing for the first 
time is getting insight into the meaning of genius. As he looks at the violin 
he is trying to understand how some great composer could compose music, a 
beautiful melody, out of wood and strings and bows. How could he put 
together such music? Did he do it in his mind, or did he run his fingers over 
the strings, or with the bow? He feels that he, too, would like to compose such 
beautiful melodies for the instrument. But he feels that he has a long way 
to go before he can reach such heights. This is his state of mind, which is 
encouraging for the future. One day he will learn that he can. As he draws 
his bow over the strings an entirely new melody comes, simple and beautiful, 
and he realizes that this is something at least akin to what the great artists 
have done. This is only the beginning, and seems to have come spontaneously, 
through no effort of his own. He can't explain how. And several things might 
happen. Possibly he could become a composer, or perhaps only come to realize 
that everyone can create beautiful music, and that this creation is only the 
energy one has in them. Music might come from everyone. It might not be 
beautiful music but it might be an expression of what was hi people. So the 
man dreams about music and his experience, out of love of music, will give 
him more understanding into life and into his fellow men. 

Or work to excite applause. 

She had always wanted to act, but she had been a poor girl and didn't think 
that she would ever get the opportunity to study drama. As a young girl she 
worked hard to help the family along financially. But every chance she got she 
would study drama. She would stay awake reading all the books she could 
get her hands on, long after all of the rest of the family were asleep. And 
she finally did get a chance to go and try out for a play. It was her big chance 
and she knew it. It wasn't a big part, but if she could do it perfectly she 
would be given another chance, no doubt So she sat in her room practicing 
and re-acting her lines until she had them down pat. The day came and the 
performance was given The stage was set and there she was, seated in a 
chair, in a pensive mood. The play went along smoothly and although she 
was nervous in the beginning she was confident of her part. And she did 



238 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

magnificently. As the curtain went down, she heard a roar of applause. She 
was complimented after the play on her brilliant performance. 

Or to excel a rival. 

These men went to school together when they were young. They're rivals, 
always have been. They were always the first two in their classes and each 
worked hard to be first. This continued all their lives. Whatever one had, 
the other worked to get something better. Whatever one did, the other had to 
do better. They're meeting now and talk about calling a truce. They will 
both agree that this is childish. But it won't work. The minute they separate, 
they will be up to their old tricks, each working to outdo the other. 

Or to be as good as another person. 

This little boy admires his father and wants to be as great a violinist as his 
father is. He can play a little bit and he realizes how much effort it will take 
to be as good as his father. But he will work hard and practice. And some- 
day he will be as good as his father. 

Or he may wish to work with others. 

This boy is blind, and has been blind since birth. His parents are quite 
musical. In fact, they are so wrapped up in their music and in each 
other that they have had little time to spend on the boy during his forma- 
tive years. They cared about him enough to see that a competent person 
was always around to see that he did not get into trouble. But they themselves 
gave him little attention. He has now gotten old enough to be curious about 
his parents and they, not understanding him, have hit upon the idea of teaching 
him how to play the violin. They have given him the instrument, had him 
feel the parts of the violin with their guidance, and now have left him alone 
with it. The boy was quite happy when they were with him, but now he 
feels that once again he has been thrown on his own. He feels that he can 
play the instrument but he would get much more enjoyment out of playing 
it with them. So now he is deciding what to do. He will decide upon the 
simplest procedure. He will pick up the instrument and make a few sounds 
on it. But he knows that they will lose interest in him and his progress and 
that, once again, he will have to fall back on the companionship of the person 
who looks after him. 

Or he may wish to work to help others by means of his skill. 

We'll make a doctor out of him. Albert had dedicated all his lif e toward 
becoming a physician. From very early days he felt a sympathy for things, for 
beings in distress, in pain. There was die time when he healed the sick squirrel 
back to health. And the time that he reset the front paw of Fido, the fox 
terrier pup of one of his neighbors' children. Always in his heart was the 
desire to make people and animals happy and well. Pain and deformities seemed 
to him out of pkce. During his high school years he seriously decided to study 
medicine. Then came the hard years through college and medical school, and 
finally he hung out his shingle. Late the first night he started practice, 



DIAGNOSIS OF PERSONALITY: WORK. SETTING 



239 



a neighbor called him to visit his sick wife who appeared to be dying in an 
asthmatic attack. Albert raced over, and after a long time it seemed that he 
was about to lose his first case. Nothing he did seemed to do any good. He 
almost despaired and was about to give up, when realizing that a human 
life was at stake and that he had to save it. He went back to work again with 
renewed faith in himself and his efforts. Shortly afterwards, the woman 
began to regain her breath her natural color, after her hard struggle and 
almost suffocation. With a sigh of contentment, Albert mopped his brow and 
thanked God for not allowing him to fail in the first great test of service. 

Or he may wish to repay others. 

The young girl is looking at her family plowing in the fields. She knows that 
they have worked hard all their lives and have spent all their money to send 
her to school so that she will not have to go through all this, too. She is 
determined that she will educate herself so that some day she will become a 
great woman and repay them for all they have done for her, so that they will 
never have to toil in the hot fields again. She of course will achieve her goal 
and make for her family a house of luxury where they can sit and waste their 
time away as other women of riches have done. 

Or to help others to a fuller life. 

The impression received is that of a young girl returning from school and 
stopping to refresh her memory with the fact that these people are hard- 
working people, people who have to live close to nature and have material 
and practical things, not enjoying many, if any, of the pleasures of life. It is 
her ingrained hope that through close attention and diligence to her studies 
she will be able someday to help these people, particularly her own, to a life 
filled with a few more pleasures. 

Or work may be a means to spiritual fulfillment. 

This is an ambitious girl, a dreamer. But not merely a dreamer. She accom- 
plishes much because of her determination. She was born in poor circum- 
stances but she had made headway. She is planning to go to another city. 
She wants to see the world. She has great dreams to fashion. But her heart 
is tied to the soil. In fact, the best work she will ever accomplish will be in 
connection with the people of the villages, the farmers, the "tillers, the down- 
trodden. Here she is thinking of a beautiful poem she could write about these 
rude folk. She is inspired by the sight of the soil and the man at work. How- 
ever, the sight of the fine strong-muscled man also stirs in her a different 
emotion and she feels the inadequacy of her personal life. The woman at the 
back symbolizes this sex-urge, which has the power to defeat this girl's other 
life. The expression on the symbol, the woman's face, suggests how confident she 
is of triumphing over the girl. The girl thinks what terrific happiness it would 
mean to gratify this desire and be a mother of children and serve the needs 
of this strong man. But she also realizes that such happiness would be 
transient and that soon she would yearn for spiritual fulfillment. She thinks 
of Madame Curie and others who have accomplished much through denying 



240 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

themselves, and she thus manages to control herself. She eventually becomes 
a great poet. 

Or happiness may be found only in work. 

He's restless. It's night and he's through working for the day but he can hardly 
wait for tomorrow to come. He's fascinated by what he*s doing and he's 
only completely happy when he's working. His work for him is his whole 
life. 

Or work may be the means to future security. 

These are two patient old people. They've worked hard all their lives. They 
are not exactly misers but theyVe worked hard and saved so that they would 
have security in their old age. Now the time has come for them to stop 
working. They will spend the twilight years of their lives together in peace. 

Or the hero may work to achieve power and money. 

There was once a young scientist in the late nineteenth century who became 
a genius in the use of explosive chemicals. He loved a young girl very dearly 
and married her. They were married a great many years, during which time 
he received great amounts of money from various countries. But the various 
explosives he invented were for uses of destruction. He gradually acquired a 
great desire, or lust, for money and power and he continued to invent these 
deadly explosives, each one more powerful and deadly than the other. But 
beneath this lust was imbedded his love for this girl for whom he thought he 
was doing best by showering on her these fabulous riches. Day after day, she 
pleaded with him to give up this business and devote his time to medical 
science instead. But his lust grew until, one day, after having made a 
terrifically explosive powder, he left it in his laboratory while he went off to 
negotiate for its sale. While he was gone, for some unknown reason which 
we can attribute to nature, this powder exploded, blowing up the house and 
killing his wife. Though still a young man, the loss of his wife suddenly 
made him realize the power of nature and his wrongdoing. The loss of his 
wife affected him so that within the next few years he aged almost twenty 
years. He gave up his malicious lust and turned his ability to medical science. 
Day after day, he visited the grave of his wife, until finally he died an old man 
with great accomplishments in the medical field behind him and a full realiza- 
tion of what his lust had cost him. 

Or to raise his socio-economic status. 

Mike was a poor boy who early in life decided that he was going to climb 
the ladder from the lowest to the highest rung, a typical Horatio Alger. He 
started with the usual job a paper route. Whatever there was to do he was 
a jump ahead of anyone else. He didn't waste time looking at rich kids and 
wishing he could be like them, he worked to be sure he would be. People 
liked him for his frankness and rung by rung he climbed the ladder until he 
finally stood at the top. He married the boss's daughter and settled down to raise 
a bunch of kids like the ones he never had time to envy. 



DIAGNOSIS OF PERSONALITY: WORK SETTING 24! 

Or he may work to earn his living. 

A haggard young transient had been searching for the past few years for a 
steady job. He was not looking for a handout but a good hard job where 
he could earn his own Jiving. But his weak, haggard appearance prevented 
any employer from hiring him for the least position. But one day, while 
walking near a river, he saw the entire outfit of some well-dressed man who 
had discarded them. He put them on, and with a clean-shaved appearance he 
returned to town to look for a job, and there rose rapidly because of his 
dignified appearance and ability to work hard. He saw that it was clothes 
that made the man. 

Or because of a cathexis for money per se. 

This is the story of a miser. All he wants in life is money. That's all he 
cares about. He slaves for years and carefully carries the money home and 
adds it to his growing hoard. He loves to handle it and count it This goes 
on for years and every week he cashes his pay check and adds it to his 
hoard. Then he dies. The moral of this story is, "You can't take it with you." 

Or to earn money for marriage. 

He's an artist and although he does his best he can't seem to earn any money. 
He's got to earn money or this girl's family won't let her marry him. He 
works day and night hoping to paint something that he can sell. Without his 
knowing about it she takes one of his pictures and enters it in a contest like 
the Pepsi-Cola contest or whatever it is. She doesn't tell him anything about 
it and he wins a prize of $5,000 which is enough for them to get married on. 

This is a sample of the more common meanings of work as reflected in 
TAT stories. 

B. LEVEL ANALYSIS 

Our concern here is with the sequence of levels characteristic of the work 
of the individual. One of the more important relationships to be examined 
is that between the level of daydream and the level of behavior. 

There are those for whom the daydream is a tonic spurring the individual 
on and sustaining his work. This is seen in the following story. 

The girl is daydreaming. She wants to become a great artist, the kind that 
people admire. She is a very smart girl and she wants to make a name for 
herself. So she studies diligently and develops into a renowned person. 

For others the daydream is either a retreat from work or so exciting that the 
intention to work is short-circuited in the daydream. 

This young kid is daydreaming about being a doctor, I mean a surgeon, and 
presumably looking forward to being one of the better known and able ones. 
Judging from the mustaches on the doctors, he may be thinking of some 



242 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

earlier pioneer in medicine operating with relatively simple tools, and rustic 
surroundings. 

Daydreaming in the next story is a retreat from work which the hero is 
unable to continue. 

It's a lazy spring night and this boy is standing at the window thinking about 
the studying he should be doing. But it's such a beautiful night he just stands 
looking out until finally he realizes that it's getting late and rouses himself 
and gets to work. He works for about an hour but then he can't seem to 
concentrate. He starts dreaming about the good time he's going to have this 
summer. He's sleepy, so he closes the books and goes to bed. 

The daydream following the esthetic experience (levels of perception and 
feeling) in some cases not only enables the individual to work more effectively 
but may constitute an important part of creative work. This may be true 
of any type of work in which the behavioral component is a relatively minor 
element, as in the following story. 

An artistic soul who likes to watch the sky [long pause]. This young man is 
very fond of music. He's worked very hard for several years as a music 
student and is especially interested in composition. During the past few 
weeks his experiences have given rise to a wide range of emotions. A brother 
was killed in action in Europe; a sister was happily married; friends have 
come to visit him. He felt unsettled and had difficulty working. He opened 
the window and watched the stars. As he pondered about the universe, his 
family, and his friends, new and beautiful melodies surged into his mind. He 
began to write furiously. All night long he worked and produced his symphony 
of night, which marked the beginning of his real career. 

The esthetic experience may renew and enrich the individual's work. 

This boy was spending the evening at home, working. As the evening wore 
on the whole atmosphere became oppressive. He went to the window to breathe 
in fresh air and as he looked out the beauty of the twilit sky overwhelmed him. 
He felt small, insignificant, and yet part of a mighty universe. The experience 
both saddened and uplifted him; he felt purified and returned to his work 
with a feeling of having found his perspective. His work acquired a heightened 
quality thereby. 

The level of daydream, as such, may have differential effects, depending 
on the specific content of the phantasy. In the following two stories, told by 
the same individual, the daydream concerned with work is a tonic to further 
work, but the daydream of love disrupts work. 

This is a Harvard graduate student, and, uh, he's been burning the midnight 
oil. He's studying some science like anthropology and he starts dreaming 
about the different peoples all over the earth. And he gets an exciting picture 
of all these peoples blended into a harmonious whole, working together for 
a better life for everyone. He's very excited and he goes back to work and 



DIAGNOSIS OF PERSONALITY: WORK SETTING 343 

works hard, with the idea that the work he's doing will enable him to do his 
part in bringing his dream to reality. 

The girl in the foreground is a student on her way home from school and she 
passes this farm everyday on her way home. Today she notices that they have a 
new hired man working in the field. She stops to talk to the woman, curious, 
and hoping that she'll meet him. But he goes on working and she goes home. 
That night after supper as she's doing her homework she finds herself dream- 
ing of his strong muscular back and his nicely shaped head. Something stirs 
within her. She tells herself that this is nonsense and tries to get back to 
work. But she can't seem to. She begins to plan ways she can get to meet him. 

The content of the phantasy, however, may not affect the outcome. Any 
type of phantasy may disrupt work, or work may continue no matter what 
the content of the phantasy. In the following stories told by an individual 
compulsive in his work, we see an example of the latter. 

A very ambitious man, a junior executive in some big firm. He's having 
lunch with the vice-president who's telling him how pleased he is with his 
work, and to keep it up and someday soon he'll get a promotion. By the time 
he gets back to the office he's walking on air and sits at his desk dreaming of 
the day when he, too, will be a vice-president But he saaps himself out of it 
with the thought that there's a lot of work to be done before then and starts 
to write a report on a big account he's just gotten. 

A young man who's very hard-working. He's brought some work home from 
the office to do. He's interested in getting ahead. He works for awhile and 
tjien goes to the window to get some air. He looks out at the city below. 
He's thinking of his girl and how much he'd like to be out with her tonight. 
Then he shuts the window. There'll be other nights and he has work to do. 

This is, in fact, a person compulsive in his work behavior. It makes no 
difference whether he dreams of promotion or of an evening of pleasure with 
his girl. The daydream yields to work in both cases. 

The following story, however, shows the opposite trend: phantasy, no 
matter what its content, is disruptive to the activity of the individual. 

"We are all of us dreamers of dreams." That's what Anne thought, as she 
passed her sister leaning against a tree. She was probably dreaming about the 
new baby that was coining, what it would look like and what ir would say. 
Other people dreamed and Anne couldn't understand why she was always 
being told to keep her mind on what she was doing, and not to dream so 
much. But she couldn't help it. The pictures in her history book brought 
dreams of the days of old and the Kings and Queens who had ruled then. She 
forgot to do her lessons. When she was supposed to feed the chickens she 
would find herself trying to hear what they said to each other. And it was 
time to go to school before she remembered to feed them. When she was sent 
to get water from the well she would imagine that it was a wishing well and 
all she had to do was to- make a wish and it would come true. One morning 
she was in Bagdad before she heard her sister callincr from the house. It was 



244 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

worse in school. The clouds made such beautiful pictures that she couldn't 
turn her eyes from the window. And one day the teacher spoke to her three 
times before she turned from the window. And she had to stay after school. 
It would be so nice if people would try to understand and not keep after her. 
But she guessed that when people got as old as her sister and teacher they were 
just like that 

Another level of psychological function important for the work process 
is the level of thought. Individuals differ significantly in the extent to which 
their work is governed by conceptual activity. For some individuals such 
activity typically precedes work. Others prefer initial trial and error activity 
and only later turn to the conceptual level for further clarification. Such 
reflection may or may not lead to further work. 

In the following story the hero, initially uncertain of his competence 
goes to work and discovers that he is in fact incompetent. He then turns to 
the level of thought. His "pondering" does not, however, lead to further work. 

This boy is looking at the violin. He is thinking wondering whether he will 
ever be able to play it. He started playing the violin and found that the sound 
produced was not as perfect as the sound he had heard from other violins. 
The outcome is that he has set the violin down on the table with the bow and 
is now pondering what he can do to make the sound coming from the violin 
mofe perfect. 

Yet, as we see in the next story, such withdrawal to the level of thought 
may bear fruit in further work and ultimate success. 

It's a boy contemplating a violin. More than likely he has been practicing. 
Having found difficulty with the sheet in front of him, he has placed his violin 
down and wonders why he can't produce the music that he knows should be 
gotten out of it I say he has been playing because of the position of the 
violin. On the whole, his attitude is one of study and wonderment More 
than likely he will resume his playing of the instrument Having spent time 
studying it once, he will go back with renewed determination to finish the 
score before him. As a result of this interest and contemplative study of his, 
he is probably headed to be a good musician. It is quite possible that he may 
become one of the world's greatest violinists or composers because he has 
shown the qualities of consideration of his instrument, contemplation and an 
attempt to understand his instrument which brings about a love of the instru- 
ment and greater productivity, all of which point to a productive later life. 
Other facts, he's well groomed, his hair is neat, and the care with which 
he has put his bow on the table, indicate later success. 

The level of thought may, however, lead neither to a dead end nor to 
further work and success but rather to confidence in future work and success. 

A young boy looks out of his open window into the moonlit sky. He is a 
college student and this night he went to bed. But his mind was not tired. 
He was working vigorously. First he turned over in his mind the lecture he 



DIAGNOSIS OF PERSONALITY: WORK. SETTING 245 

had heard during the day. Little bits of each one came to him, exciting intel- 
lectual thoughts, but his mind leaped from one to the other. Regarding each 
he thought, "This I must know more about" Then he began to think of 
when he would learn all these things, when he would find the answers, how 
he would find the answers to the thrilling and exciting problems that arose 
every day. At this point he got up, went over to 'the window. He looked 
off into space as though somehow the breadth of the vista before him might 
find him some perspective. He thought of what he might do, what career he 
might choose. He did not think of people. He thought only of ideas. At this 
moment he felt himself a kind of intellectual giant in whose hand lay the 
whole world. He would learn about it, he would know all about it, he 
would have power over it 

C. DIFFERENTIATION OF MEANS AND ENDS 

Does the individual differentiate between means and end? The nature 
of the work may be such that it cannot be broken down into means and end. 
This is especially true of work which offers no particular problems; either 
it is simple and never required much training, or it is complex but has become 
habitual and presents no further problems. For the creation of such a dis- 
tinction there usually has to be some discrepancy between the wish and the 
achievement. No distinction is made between means and ends in the following 
story because of the routine nature of the work. 

Her first year as secretary to Mr. Jones was drawing to a close. Somehow she 
wished she had never wanted to take a white collar job. How could she have 
ever wanted to work, 'work, work. Up at 6:30. How? The alarm of 
course. Turn on the electric lights, make toast on the toaster, go to work on 
an electric trolley, go to the i6th floor by elevator and work all day on the 
typewriter, adding machines, machines, electricity, everything mechanical. 
Everything is scheduled, nothing free, natural, unhurried from 6:30 to 5:30 
every day, day in, day out Why, you'd think she was a mechanical man, or 
robot, or something. Oh, to get away from it all. Never to have to even set 
the alarm clock, much less turn it off. To be free to do as you pleascl Never 
to have to catch the 8:30 bus. Thank heavens her vacation was so near. 
Perhaps she might quit this job. Joyce had spoken of another job that had 
sounded like less monotony. Yes, that was what she wanted to do. Mr. 
Jones was disappointed to hear her proposal. She was an attractive receptionist, 
efficient, and had become completely familiar with the routine of his office. To 
train a new girl, and most likely one not as good, would take several months. 
So it was with this in mind that he gave her the chance to come back after 
her vacation if she should happen to reconsider. The vacation was ideal. She 
and Joyce went together and just basked in the sunshine, not caring what 
hour of the day and without a care in the world It was a change all right 
and at first it was heaven. But somehow the contrast to a hard working day 
seemed too great and was not as appealing to her as she had thought it would 
be. By the end of her vacation Anne realized that she really wanted and 
craved some sort of routine and planned work. She had always loved a sense 
of accomplishment and actually craved a certain amount of it So it was with 



246 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

eagerness that she went back to Mr. Jones and remained his secretary for 
many years. 

In the next story, however, means and ends are differentiated. We see 
that the conditions necessary for this distinction exist; there is a difference 
between the hero's desire to be a doctor and his achievement of this end. 

We'll make a- doctor out of him. Albert had dedicated all his life toward 
becoming a physician. From very early days, he felt a sympathy for things, 
for beings in distress, in pain. There was the time when he healed the sick 
squirrel back to health and the time that he reset the front paw of Fido, the 
fox terrier pup of one of his neighbors* children. Always in his heart was the 
desire to make people and animals happy and well. Pain and deformities seemed 
to him out of place. During his high school years he seriously decided to 
study medicine. Then came the hard years through college and medical school 
and finally he hung out his shingle. Late the first night he started practice, a 
neighbor called him to visit his sick wife who appeared to be dying in an 
asthmatic attack. Albert raced over, and after a long time, it seemed that he 
was about to lose his first case. Nothing he did seemed to do any good. He 
almost despaired, and was about to give up, when realizing that a human 
life was at stake and that he had to save it. He went back to work with 
renewed faith in himself and his efforts. Shortly afterward, the woman began 
to regain her breath, her natural color, after her hard struggle and almost 
suffocation. With a sigh of contentment, Albert mopped his brow and thanked 
God for not allowing him to fail in this first great test of service. 

If there is a differentiation between means and end, to what extent is 
this differentiation elaborated? 

D. MEANS-END COGNIZANCE 

An examination of the protocol for means-end cognizance is useful in 
assessing the realism of the individual in the work situation. Does he wish or 
expect to achieve his ends through means-end activity or does he fail to con- 
sider the necessary means? 

The hero may possess no means-end cognizance. 

A little boy, about six who is dreaming of being a very great and wonderful 
violinist, one who leaves his audience spellbound with the power and beauty of 
his music. And then we see him years later receiving a tremendous ovation, 
his dream come true. 

Or he may possess some means-end cognizance. 

The boy is gazing at the violin with a dreamy, rapt expression on his face. He 
is a music lover and hopes someday to be a great musician. He knows that it 
means hard work but he will accomplish his aim and play in all the great 
cities all over the world. 



DIAGNOSIS OF PERSONALITY: WORK, SETTING 247 

Or he may possess a high degree of means-end cognizance. 

It's a boy contemplating a violin. More than likely he has been practicing. 
Having found difficulty with the sheet in front of him, he has placed his 
violin down and wonders why he can't produce the music that he knows should 
be gotten out of it I say he has been playing because of the position of the 
violin. On the whole his attitude is one of study and wonderment. More 
than likely he will resume his playing of the instrument. Having spent time 
studying it once, he will go back with renewed determination to finish the 
score before him. As a result of this interest and contemplative study of his, 
he is probably headed to be a good musician. It is quite possible that he may 
become one of the world's greatest violinists or composers because he has shown 
the qualities of consideration of his instrument, contemplation and an attempt 
to understand his instrument which brings about a love of the instrument 
and greater productivity, all of which point to a productive later life. Other 
facts, he's well groomed, his hair is neat, and the care with which he has put 
his bow on the table indicate later success. 

The highest degree of means-end cognizance, however, is found where 
there is greatest elaboration of means. This is seen in the following story 
told by a business executive. 

This looks like a scene from the Lost World. A number of explorers have 
been investigating this territory perhaps in the vicinity of New Mexico. They 
come upon this path through a canyon. There doesn't seem to be any living 
thing whatever in the entire, vicinity. Everything is one monotonous color, 
a sort of dirty, brownish ivory. In a little while their eyes become accustomed 
to the light and coloration and notice various shapes one in particular of a 
dinosaur, seems to be dormant, but its outspread webbed foot seems to indicate 
that it may be alive. As they approach a natural bridge, toward the dinosaur, 
they are confronted with a spider-like creature or a bug-like creature, except 
that it appears to be gigantic compared with an ordinary bug. This, too, this 
is moving. The creature however seems to pay no attention.. They're fearful 
of going on and decide to return with weapons and some means of carrying 
out these creatures if they could be captured. Strangely enough when they 
return a day and a half later both creatures are in the same spot as they had 
previously been. The bug still moved about and the men advanced slowly. The 
bug's horrible face and ugly legs put fear in the men's hearts. They decide 
not to capture him alive. A number of men let fly with high powered rifles. 
They aimed for his head and all shot accurately. The bug crumbled to dust 
By some strange phenomenon the injury to his head disintegrated the entire 
body. The men were extremely disappointed and downcast. They decided 
not to try the same tactics with the dinosaur if he were alive. They approached 
warily, but the dinosaur made no move They got closer and closer to him until 
they were able to throw a lasso around his neck. They carefully tied it in a 
slip knot and all the men took hold of the rope and with one at a signal, with 
one great tug, they pulled the monster from his place. He came very easily* 
There was no weight to him at all. As a matter of fact he was just a hollow 



248 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

shell. The back half of him crumbled as he hit the rocks. They rushed for- 
ward in an effort to save what was left His body had been broken in too 
many pieces and they decided to cut up certain pieces which appeared that they 
would break in transportation. They transported the animal out in 45 wooden 
cases. It would probably take years to assemble him again. Took only one 
case to assemble the remains of the bug. They would get to that problem 
later. Perhaps they would let some of their assistants experiment with the dust 
They had not originally expected a find as extraordinary as this, but now that 
they had found it and seemingly not captured these creatures correctly, they 
were a little bit disappointed. Their disappointment was forgotten in the 
years that they labored on reconstruction, and they looked with pride on their 
accomplishment when their creature was unveiled to the public at the museum 
of natural history. 

Such a degree of means-end cognizance may, however, have pathological 
sequelae. This is seen in the following story where the hero's high degree 
of means-end cognizance paralyzes action. 

In the foreground is a young boy and I guess what's above is his dream a 
dream about being a doctor. This dream comes from a book he's read about 
doctors, Arrowsmith or something like that and he wants to be a doctor. He's 
thinking about the years of hard, grueling work entailed. He's have to study 
biology and chemistry and physics in college and then go to medical school 
for years, and learn anatomy, physiology, surgery and all the other sciences 
and then he'll have to be an interne and take care of people in hospitals and 
then be a resident in a hospital. It would be very hard work and take years 
and years. He never docs become a doctor. 

MEANS-END ELABORATION IN DAYDREAMING AND BEHAVIOR 

We cannot assume, however, that because we find no means-end elabora- 
tion on the level of daydreaming that this is also true on the level of behavior. 
It may be that the absence of means-end cognizance on the level of phantasy 
merely represents a short-cut between wish and achievement not found on the 
level of behavior. To ascertain whether or not this is so, it is necessary to 
compare the individual's work stories on the level of daydreaming and the 
level of behavior. In the following two stories from the same protocol we find 
an example of this. The first story will be recognized as one in which the hero 
possesses no means-end cognizance on the. level of daydreaming: 

A little boy, about six, who is daydreaming of being a very great and wonderful 
violinist, one who leaves his audience spellbound with die power and beauty 
of his music. And then we sec him years later receiving a tremendous ovation, 
his dream come true. 

In the next story, however, the hero possesses a high degree of means end 
cognizance on the level of behavior. 



DIAGNOSIS OF PERSONALITY! WORK SETTING 249 

These men went to school together when they were young. They're rivals, 
always have been. They were always the first two in their classes and each 
worked hard to be first. This continued all their lives. Whatever one had, the 
other worked to get something better. Whatever one did, the other had to do 
better. They're meeting now and talk about calling it a truce. They will both 
agree that this is childish. But it won't work. The minute they separate, they 
will be up to their old tricks, each working to outdo the other. 

It is important to note that at the beginning of these two stories the heroes 
are approximately the same age. The factor of age must be controlled in this 
type of comparison. It would otherwise be impossible to tell whether the dif- 
ference in means-end cognizance is a function of the difference in the age of 
the hero or a function of the level of the story. 

E. CATHEXIS OF MEANS 

Cathexis of means may be either positive or negative. TJie hero of the 
story which follows found the means so arduous that he abandoned his goal. 

The little boy wants to learn to play the violin and he's looking at it and 
wondering if he ever will be able to. It looks so complicated and the music 
looks hard. He'll have to learn to read music and then to make the music 
come out on the strings with the bow and then he'll have to learn to play 
scales. Then he'll have to practice everyday for hours. He's very discouraged 
and decides maybe he doesn't want to learn. 

Whereas the hero of the next story is positively cathected to the means. 

This man is very young. Ever since he was a small child he has been interested 
in music. He has never been able to express his feelings about it but he has 
always wanted to take lessons. His favorite instrument is the violin. He has 
begun to take lessons and he likes it very much. As he first begins to under- 
take his instrument, after he has studied a small while, he realizes the difficulty 
of what he has undertaken and compares his playing with that of the old 
masters. He realizes how different it is. What he is learning and doing and 
for the first time he is getting insight into the meaning of genius. As he looks 
at the violin he is trying to understand how some great composer could compose 
music, a beautiful melody, out of wood and strings and bows. How could he 
put together such music? Did he do it in his mind or did he run his fingers 
over the strings or with the bow? He feels that he, too, would like to compose 
such beautiful melodies for the instrument But he feels that he has a long 
way to go before he can reach such heights. This is his state of mind, which 
is encouraging for the future. One day he will learn that he can. As he draws 
his bow over the strings an entirely new melody comes, simple and beautiful, 
and he realizes that this is something -at least akin to what the great artists 
have done. This is only the beginning and seems to have come spontaneously 
through no effort of his own. He can't explain how. And several things might 
happen. Possibly he could become a composer, or perhaps only come to realize 



250 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

that everyone can. create beautiful music and that this creation is only the 
energy one has in them. Music might come from everyone. It might not be 
beautiful music but might be an expression of what was in people. So the 
man dreams about music, and his experience out of the love of music will 
give him more understanding into life and into his fellow men. 

I. MEANS WHICH BECOME ENDS 

A familiar phenomenon in work is the transformation of means into ends. 
Any activity which is pursued for a long period of time may eventually become 
an end in itself, although it was originally a means to some other end. Thus 
the hero of the following story worked originally in order to support his family, 
but a lifetime of such work renders him incapable of retiring from work when 
there is no longer the same reason for continuing it. 

This fellow has worked hard all his life. He's raised a big family and it 
wasn't easy meeting all the bills but he kept at it day in and day out, never 
took a vacation but somehow he never seemed to mind it in fact he used to 
laugh to himself when his family pitied him, for the hours he had to work he 
really enjoyed it and when he got old enough to retire he wouldn't. He had 
plenty of money now, but he just couldn't see himself sitting around doing 
nothing. 

2. ACTIVITY VALUED AS MEANS TO AN END 

The individual may, however, cathect means only as they serve another 
end. The end may be something completely outside the realm of work, as in 
the following story where the hero reforms and starts working, "so that if he 
falls in love he won't have to wait for a good job." 

This is a young man who lives an ordinary life without any particular ambition. 
All he cares about is getting enough money to live. One night just before he 
goes to bed, he turns off the lights and is looking out of his window. He 
realizes he has spent a good deal of his life with no purpose and it is time 
to settle down. It is not because his conscience troubles him but just because 
lie feels he is missing something in his life. He needs someone to love him 
and to love. He wishes now that he had something to offer a girl if he fell 
in love with the right one. He decides that now he will reform and start 
really working so that if he falls in love he won't have to wait for a good job. 

Or the means may be cathected as a means to an end within the realm of work. 

The girl is daydreaming. She wants to become a great artist, the kind that 
people admire. She is a very smart girl and she wants to make a name or 
reputation for herself so she studies diligently and develops into a renowned 
person. 

3. ENJOYMENT OF BARRIER SOLUTION 

Certain aspects of the total work process may be cathected. One of these 
is the barrier which presents itself before the end may he attained. 



DIAGNOSIS OF PERSONAUTY: WORK SETTING 25! 

Thus, although the hero of the following story is primarily interested in 
making money from his business, he derives pleasure in breaking down the 
sales resistance of a particularly difficult client. 

The young serious man is trying to sell the other man insurance. This is a 
very big deal, it may mean a $100,000 policy if he can put it across. This guy 
is a tough customer but it doesn't faze him. In fact he enjoys it and prides 
himself on knowing just what to say to men like this, how to flatter them but 
not so that they get suspicious. First a little shop talk to show that he knows 
his business and then he tells them that every intelligent man should have 
insurance and quotes figures and mortality statistics. It works well with this 
one and by the time the afternoon is over the deal is closed. 

F. COMPLEXITY OF ORGANIZATION 

Does the individual attribute complexity of organization to the work of 
his heroes? Does the work involve the organization of a large number of 
parts into a whole? This is important in estimating the complexity of organiza- 
tion of work of which the individual is capable since he cannot attribute to his 
heroes a degree of complexity of organization of work which surpasses his own 
capacity. We see this in the following story, told by a gifted individual who 
attributed his own subtle intelligence to the hero. 

Mr. Pritchard was not an ordinary mathematician. His chief delight lay in 
reducing to mathematical formulae the creations of artists both in the fields of 
music and drawing and of reproducing these conveniently according to the 
formulae which he had derived. His system was the old one of reducing lines 
to multiples of simple wave forms. He could reproduce a face with as few as 
fifty multiples of a sine wave and had even developed methods for doing 
landscapes. But, as he grew older, he became more and more enchanted with 
his own success, and seized on every opportunity to make pictures of figures. 
Finally he conceived the idea of finding the hidden meanings in the Old 
Testament by developing a formula including all the numbers quoted there 
and then transforming it to a picture. This he did. The result being a huge 
drawing containing sections which were more or less units in themselves. It 
was one of these that finally drove him mad. It was the picture of an old man 
riding on the shoulders of another, goading him ever onwards. He read into 
this the story of his own defeat 

G. TEMPORAL CHARACTERISTICS OF WORK 

I. TIME PERSPECTIVE PLANNING 

Time perspective in work must be differentiated from complexity of 
organization. An individual may possess a long time perspective and care- 
fully plan his work over a long period of time but the work may be simple 
and lack complexity of organization. These two dimensions may, however, 



252 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

be combined, as in the following story where the work is complex, well 
organized, and planned over a long period of time. 

This looks like a scene from the Lost World. A number of explorers have 
been investigating this territory perhaps in the vicinity of New Mexico. They 
come upon this path through a canyon. There doesn't seem to be any living 
thing whatever in the entire vicinity. Everything is one monotonous color, a 
sort of dirty, brownish, ivory. In a litde while their eyes become accustomed 
to the light and coloration and notice various shapes one in particular of a 
dinosaur, seems to be dormant, but its outspread webbed foot seems to indicate 
that it may be alive. As they approach a natural bridge toward the dinosaur 
they are confronted with a spider-like creature or a bug-like creature, except 
that it appears to be gigantic compared with an ordinary bug. This too, this 
is moving. The creature, however, seems to pay no attention. They're fearful 
of going on and decide to return with weapons and some means of carrying 
out these creatures if they could be captured. Strangely enough when they 
return a day and a half later both creatures are in the same spot spots, as they 
had previously been. The bug still moved about and the men advanced slowly. 
The bug's horrible face and ugly legs put fear in the men's hearts. They 
decided not to capture him alive. A number of men let fly with high powered 
rifles. They aimed for his head and all shot accurately. The bug crumbled to 
dust. By some strange phenomenon the injury to his head disintegrated the 
entire body. The men were extremely disappointed and downcast. They 
decided not to try the same tactics with the dinosaur if he were alive. They 
approached warily, but the dinosaur made no move. They got closer and 
closer to him until they were able to throw a lasso around his neck. They 
carefully tied it in a slip knot and all the men took hold of the rope and with 
one at a signal, with one great tug. They pulled the monster from his place. 
He came very easily. There was no weight to him at all. As a matter of fact 
he was just a hollow shell. The back half of him crumbled as he hit the 
rocks. They rushed forward in an effort to save what was left. His body 
had been broken in too many pieces and they decided to cut up certain pieces 
which appeared that they would break in transportation. They transported 
the animal out in 45 wooden cases. It would probably take years to assemble 
him again. Took only one case to assemble the remains of the bug. They 
would get to that problem later. Perhaps they would let some of their 
assistants experiment with the dust They had not originally expected a find 
as extraordinary as this but now that they had found it and seemingly not 
captured these creatures correctly they were a little bit disappointed. Their 
disappointment was forgotten in the years that they labored on reconstruction, 
and they looked with pride on their accomplishment when their creature 
was unveiled to the public at the museum of natural history. 

2. ENDURANCE 

How long does the work actually endure? Endurance is a dimension of 
the work region which varies independently of time perspective. The hero 
may have a long time perspective but his actual work may not endure for long. 
Or, as we saw in the story above, both the time perspective and the endurance 



DIAGNOSIS OF PERSONALITY: WORK SETTING 253 

may be long, or the hero, although he works for a long period of time, may 
have no time perspective. Endurance is implicit in any work story although 
it may not be mentioned directly. We see this in the following story. The 
heroine does not tell us how long the work endures, she tells us that "eventually 
her sacrifices bear fruit and she becomes a great poet." Implicit in this is the 
long duration of the work. 

This woman is crying because she is lonely. She is isolated emotionally because 
of her own peculiar personality. She is a very gifted woman and therefore 
eccentric enough to make her isolated. At the same time she would like to be 
liked, to have someone passionately fond of her, to be understood and appre- 
ciated. The people who do get attached to her are usually of a lower caliber 
while those for whom she could care always have some impediment. She has 
been looking at the twilight scene beyond and with the first coming of the 
stars she feels a tumult and breaks down in tears. Immediately, however, she 
masters herself and resolves to brave life. She fortifies herself with the thought 
that suffering is necessary in order to accomplish anything great Eventually 
her sacrifices bear fruit and she becomes a great poet. 

The individual, however, may describe directly the endurance of the work. 
The following stories illustrate varying periods of endurance. Work may 
endure the greater part of the hero's life. 

This elderly scientist who, having spent the greater part of his life in search 
of a means of prolonging youth is at the point of this picture about to culminate 
his work with this great experiment . . . His whole theory is based on the 
fact that organic decay can be combated by a state of mind. He fails in his 
work and dies realizing that the only thing man is sure of no, no, no that 
man can pass on is the will to do good in the eyes of God and cannot in any 
way violate the laws of nature. 

Or it may have a medium period of endurance. 

Well, this is a young man who has been working for a couple of years, and ah, 
he has been doing very much run-of-the-mill desk work. Life has been pretty 
dull. He has really gone to the office and sat there, been more or less of a 
stenographer. He has ideas of wanting to get ahead. His pictures are prettv 
good. After two years of that drudgery, he is given a promotion. Somebody 
thinks his ideas are pretty good and he is given the opportunity to work on 
the advertising copy, wording, drawing, advertising, what he thinks is pretty 
interesting. He is pretty happy about it. He has spent more time being happy 
about things than actually accomplishing much. He has gone home after a 
party. He's been to a party and he has gone home from a party he's been to 
and thought he had better accept this thing. He likes his work, and so he sits 
down at his desk and starts work . . . and somehow it is a much better time 
for work than the office where everyone is watching, and he expects to accom- 
plish something. It is more like a vision. He draws out the picture, carefully 
figures out the coloring and where it will go and where the wording will go* 
works on that until it gets pretty well smudged, takes a new piece of paper, 



254 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

tries to make it the final copy, figures out where the letters will go and the 
words and the heading. It's in sort of a diagonal heading. At the bottom he 
puts something fine, almost poetic, and gets it down. And he is very happy 
over it when he gets it done, happier than he was when he got the promotion 
and he thinks about it and what he will say about it the next day when he will 
try to sell it. He looks and sees that it is about five o'clock. It's getting light 
outside. He goes to the bathroom and washes a bit and comes back and sees 
it is light outside and goes to the window, happier than he's ever been, and he 
stands there looking out into the light and you see him here. 

Or the hero may work only for a very short time. 

It's a lazy spring night and this boy is standing at the window thinking about 
the studying he should be doing. But it's such a beautiful night he just 
stands looking out until finally he realizes that it's getting kte and rouses 
himself and gets to work. He works for about an hour but then he can't 
seem to concentrate. He starts dreaming about the good time he's going to 
have this summer. He's sleepy so he closes the books and goes to bed. 

3. CHANGES IN EGO IDEAL OR WORK HABITS AS A FUNCTION OF TIME 

We have seen in the family region that the hero's relation to his parents 
or to parent surrogates may change as a function of time. The same type of 
change may occur in the work region. When analyzing the work stories of any 
individual, the age of the hero should be taken into consideration and stories 
compared to see if there is any change in ego ideal or work habits as a function 
of time. The hero of the following two stories has a different reason for work 
in childhood and adulthood: 

This little boy admires his father and wants to be as great a violinist as his 
father is. He can play a little bit and he realizes how much effort it will take 
to be as good as his father. But he will work hard, and practice and someday 
he will be as good as his father. 

He's restless, it's night and he's through working for the day but he can 
hardly wait for tomorrow to come. He's fascinated by what he's doing and 
he's only completely happy when he's working. His work for him is his whole 
life! 

Thus we see that as a child and as an adult the hero is hard-working. 
However, the child regards the work as effort and works to "be as great a 
violinist as his father is." The man, on the other hand, works because his 
happiness lies in work and for this reason work requires little "effort." 

II. RELATIVE IMPORTANCE OF THE WORK REGION 

In this section we shall apply more briefly some of those criteria used in 
the determination of the relative importance of the family region. 



DIAGNOSIS OF PERSONALITY: WORK SETTING 



A. DIRECT REFERENCE 



255 



As illustrated in the family region, the protocols may directly refer to the 
importance of any region to the individual. 

He's restless, it's night and he's through working for the day but he can't hardly 
wait for tomorrow to come. He's fascinated by what he's doing and he's only 
completely happy when he's working. His work for him is his whole life. 

B. NUMBER AND LENGTH OF STORIES 

Other criteria which may be used in evaluating the importance of this 
region are the length of the stories told about work in comparison with those 
dealing with other regions and the number of stories in which the work region 
is treated. The most striking example we have seen of the number of stories 
concerning the work region was one protocol in which work was the principle 
theme in seventeen of the twenty stories. 

C. AFFECT AND INTENSITY 

The affect and intensity in stories about this region also provide a means 
of estimating its importance. In the following story the affect towards knowl- 
edge and learning is marked. 

A young boy looks out of his open window into the moonlit sky. He is a 
college student and this night he went to bed but his mind was not tired. He 
was working vigorously. First he turned over in his mind the lecture he had 
heard during the day. Little bits of each one came to him, exciting intellectual 
thoughts, but his mind leaped from one to the other. Regarding each he 
thought, 'This "I must know more about" Then he began to think of when 
he would learn all these things, when he would find the answers, how he 
would find the answers to the thrilling and exciting problems that arose every 
day. At this point he got up, went over to the window. He looked off into 
space as though somehow the breadth of the vista before him might find frim 
some perspective. He thought of what he might do, what career he might 
choose. He did not think of people. He thought only of ideas. At this moment 
he felt himself a kind of intellectual giant in whose hand lay* the whole world. 
He would learn about it, he would know all about it, he would have power 
over it 

D. INTRODUCTION OF STRIVERS 

We may also obtain this information by examining the way in which the 
individual treats the figures in the pictures. Are strivers introduced? This 
question may be asked about those pictures which present passive figures. In 
the picture of the cowboys stretched out on the grass, a cowboy may not be 



256 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

seen as the hero but someone who is characterized as a striver may be intro- 
duced, as in the story that follows. 

It's been a very tough climb for these men. They had been going up toward 
the summit of the peak since early this morning. And now at noon they were 
exhausted enough for a little sleep. One man who seems to have a little more 
energy than the rest has probably had enough of a stop and is anxious to get 
on, but seeing the others reposing so peacefully he does not awaken them. 

E. INTERPRETATION OF AMBIGUOUS FIGURES 

Are ambiguous figures interpreted as strivers or failers rather than as 
sinners or lovers? Ambiguous figures in this sense are those figures in the 
picture who are not engaged in any particular activity. Such interpretation 
would indicate that the individual's thinking about people tended to be in 
terms of work rather than any other value. This is illustrated in the story 
following. 

Looks like a girl who lives on a farm has just come home from school and is 
going to start work on the farm or is going to school and is watching her 
parents working Looks like she's thinking about the work she's gotta do now 
that she's home from school. What she'll probably do now is change her clothes, 
start milking the cow or something. That's about all on that. She'll continue 
living like she's been. Going to school in the day, coming home and going to 
work. 

F. RESTRUCTURATION OF PASSIVE PICTURES 
Passive pictures may be restructured as a preparation for activity. 

These are a bunch of cowboys who have been working hard all morning. 
They've just had lunch and are resting a bit because they will have to work 
hard to get all the cattle rounded up in the afternoon. They're just resting a 
minute and will go right back to work. 

G. OMISSION OF PASSIVE FIGURES 

The individual may omit passive figures in the picture from the stories 
he tells. The following story was told to picture I2M (the picture of an older 
man bending over a younger man reclining on a couch). 

This is a man who does this for a living. He's on stage and he's giving a 
demonstration of bis powers as a hypnotist. 

H. IDENTIFICATION WITH ACTIVE FIGURES 

The individual may identify with active figures when both active and 
passive figures are present in the picture. This may be ascertained by the 
story told to picture I2M. The following is an example. 



DIAGNOSIS OF PERSONALITY: WORK SETTING 257 

This is a kindly old doctor a psychiatrist who wants to help this ,patient. He 
has been giving him some sort of therapy but it hasn't been too successful and 
he's decided to try hypnotizing him and telling him that he's going to be all 
right. He's just about to bring him out of it and he hopes it will be successful. 

I. REJECTION OF PASSIVITY 

A further criterion is the individual's affective rejection of passivity or 
his affective acceptance of activity, or of figures he sees as either active or 
passive. The following stories from the same protocol illustrate such an 
attitude. 

These men are a bunch of lazy bums. They're supposed to be working and 
they're getting paid for it, but what are they doing loafing on the job. It's a 
disgrace to the country to pay men for loafing. Somebody ought to take a 
horsewhip to them and get them back to work. 

Good people these are good hard working people. The backbone of the 
nation tilling the soil, working from sunup to sundown. Probably came over 
from the old country to make a new life for themselves and have worked hard 
to make this beautiful farm and to send their daughter to school. 

J. CONFLICT 

The relative importance of the work region may also be revealed by 
examining the protocol for stories of conflict between work and any other 
region. The degree of conflict, the way in which it is resolved, and the degree 
of residual tension afford indices of the relative importance of work and other 
regions. In the following story we see a conflict between love and work which 
is resolved in favor of the latter. 

This is an ambitious girl a dreamer. But not merely a dreamer. She accom- 
plishes much because of her determination. She was born in poor circum- 
stances but she has made headway. She is planning to go to another city she 
wants to see the world. She had great dreams to fashion, but her heart is 
tied to the soil. In feet, the best work she will ever accomplish will be in 
connection with the people of the villages, the farmers, the tillers, the down- 
trodden. Here she is thinking of a beautiful poem she could write about these 
rude folk. She is inspired by the sight of the soil and the man at work. How- 
ever, the sight of the fine, strong muscled man also stirs in her a different 
emotion and she feels the inadequacy of her personal life. The woman at the 
back symbolizes this sex-urge which has the power to defeat this girl's other 
life. The expression on the symbol, the woman's face, suggests how confident 
she is of triumphing over the girl The girl thinks what terrific happiness it 
would mean to gratify this desire and be a mother of children and serve the 
needs of this strong man. But she also realizes that such happiness would be 
transient and that soon she would yearn for spiritual fulfillment She thinks 
of Madame Curie and others who have accomplished much through denying 
themselves and she thus manages to control herself. She eventually becomes 
a great poet. 



258 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TTCST 

There is some residual tension but the importance of spiritual fulfillment 
in work provides integration for her personality, enabling her to control her 
wish for sex and love. 

K. IMPACT OF WORK ON OTHER REGIONS 

Another story by the same individual illustrates a further important 
criterion the impact of work on other regions. There may be an ego ideal 
centered on work which structures the personality and allows the individual 
to tolerate frustrations in other regions. In the following story the heroine 
is enabled through success in her career as poetess to tolerate frustration of 
her wish to be loved. 

This woman is crying because she is lonely. She is isolated emotionally because 
of her own peculiar personality. She is a very gifted woman and therefore 
eccentric enough to make her isolated. At the same time she would like to be 
liked, to have someone passionately fond of her, to be understood and appre- 
ciated. The people who do get attached to her are usually of a lower caliber 
while those for whom she could care always have some impediment. She has 
been looking at the twilight scene beyond, and with the first coming of the 
stars she feels a tumult and breaks down in tears. Immediately, however, she 
masters herself and resolves to brave life. She fortifies herself with the thought 
that suffering is necessary in order to accomplish anything great. Eventually 
her sacrifices bear fruit and she becomes a great poet 

III. THE CONDITIONS OF WORK 

In the love and sex region we undertook a systematic inquiry into the 
functional interrelationships between the dimensions of that region in an 
attempt to demonstrate that the variability of any dimension between stories 
could be treated either as the effect of differences in preceding conditions in 
two or more stories or as the cause of different effects in the stories in which 
they appeared. The application of this type of analysis to the work region 
will be illustrated here. 

The following two stories were told by the same individual to the same 
TAT card (14) within a two month period. 

Well, he's been working hard all day and on into the night. Some work that 
he isn't very much interested in. Finally he was stiff and tired and his mind 
was getting sleepy. He stretched and got up and threw open the window 
and just looked out at the world around him. He's thinking about how 
beautiful the wonderful world is. He'll go to bed, go to sleep very soon. 

Well, this is a young man who has been working for a couple of years, and ah, 
he has been doing very much run-of-the-mill desk work. Life has been pretty 
dull. He has really gone to the office and sat there, been more or less of a 
stenographer. He has ideas of wanting to get ahead. His pictures are pretty 
good. After two years of that drudgery, he is given a promotion. Somebody 



DIAGNOSIS OF PERSONALITY: WORK SETTING 259 

thinks his ideas are pretty good, and he is given the opportunity to work on 
the advertising copy, wording, drawing, advertising, what he thinks is pretty 
interesting. He is pretty happy about it He has spent more time being happy 
about things than actually accomplishing much. He has gone home after a party. 
He's been to a party and he has gone home from a party he's been to and 
thought he had better accept this thing. He likes his work, and so he 
sits down and starts work on an advertisement for perfume or something, 
and he has come in, from a party or something, about one or two o'clock 
and he sits down at his desk and starts work . . . and somehow it is a 
much better time for work than the office where everyone is watching, 
and he expects to accomplish something. It is more like a vision. He draws 
out the picture, carefully figures out the coloring and where it will go and 
where the wording will go, works on that until it gets pretty well smudged, 
takes a new piece of paper, tries to make it the final copy, figures out where 
the letters will go and the words and the heading. It's in sort of a diagonal 
heading. At the bottom he puts something fine almost poetic, and gets it 
down. And he is very happy over it when he gets it done, happier than he 
was when he got the promotion and he thinks about it and what he will say 
about it the next day when he will try to sell it. He looks and sees that it is 
about five o'clock. It's getting light outside. He goes to the bathroom and 
washes a bit and comes back and sees it is light outside and goes to the window, 
happier than he's ever been, and he stands there looking out into the light and 
you see him here. 

As we read these stories we see that the physical settings and the duration 
of the work are very much the same, in both. There are however, two striking 
differences between them, a difference in length and a difference in affect about 
work in the two stories. The first story contains a brief reference to the fact 
that the work is uninteresting; no positive affect is expressed. In the second 
story ,^ however, a minute description of the nature o the work and the work 
process is given accompanied by frequent affective comment. 

The factor responsible for this difference in affect in these two stories is 
the nature of the work. In the first story the individual tells us that he is 
doing work "he isn't much interested in." In the second story, however, "he is 
given the opportunity to work on the advertising copy, wording, drawing 
advertising, what he thinks is pretty interesting" and "he likes his work." 

We are told here that interesting work creates positive affect, and interest- 
ing work upon examination proves to be creative Work. 

These stories reveal both the conditions under which this individual will 
work and the conditions which seem to him ideal. He seems to prefer to work 
at home and at night, which "is a much better time for work than at the office 
where everyone is watching." He works hard in both stories, despite the fact 
that he is doing work in the first story which "he isn't very much interested in.'* 
But his ideal conditions for work are seen in the second story where he is doing 
work which he considers interesting creative work. It is of interest, in the light 



260 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

of these findings, that the individual who told these stories planned a career 
in creative writing. 

In the protocol of another individual we find two stories which illustrate 
the conditions under which the individual will stop work, rather than the 
ideal conditions for work. 

Workers, yes, workers, fruit pickers. They work different seasons in different 
parts of the country, picking fruit, whatever crop needs picking, I guess. They 
like the work, like picking fruit, _no one to boss them. They can go as fast 
or as slow as they like. Here they're taking a snooze before going back to 
picking. No one to tell them what they should do. 

Looks like a mother and a son. Neither looks happy, the son must have quit 
his job, guess that's it and he's telling his mother. Feels bad, he liked the 
work. Had a fight with the foreman, I guess, got mad, told him he'd be 
damned if he'd be told by him what he should do, and quit 

An examination of these stories reveals that in both stories the individual 
likes his work. Why then does he continue to work in one story and stop in 
the other? 

We find that dominance is the factor responsible. In the first story he 
tells us that there is "no one to boss them"; in the second story, however, the 
foreman tries to tell him what to do, he gets angry and quits. 

In the next two stories another individual tells us the conditions which 
sustain his work. 

This young fellow has just come home from looking for a job. He's pretty 
downcast at not finding one, particularly since he's a college graduate. He's 
been out of school for about six months. He still hasn't landed anything. 
Here his father who is evidently a man of experience is consoling his son: He 
has a great faith in the boy and is sure he will get a position of which he is 
worthy. He talks to the boy for a while comfortingly and pats him on the 
back. The boy will feel much better because his father has faith in him and 
the next day he will go out looking for a job with a new determination that 
he is going to justify his father's faith. And he will 

This takes place after the school day is over. The woman with the books is 
a discouraged school teacher. She came to teach in a little country school in 
the midwest and she was full of enthusiasm. She was going to bring modern 
educational methods to these people. But instead of being eager for them, the 
people were outraged and ridiculed all her attempts at modernization. They 
thought the way they had been taught was good enough for their children. 
Finally she couldn't stand their scoffing and ridicule any longer and decided to 
leave. 

The hero of the first story is downcast about his work at the beginning 
of the story but is filled with a new determination at the end. The heroine 
of the second story is enthusiastic about her work at the beginning and becomes 



DIAGNOSIS OF PERSONALITY: WORK SETTING 26l 

discouraged. The factor responsible for this difference is the attitude of others 
whether they reject the individual or have faith in him. In the first story, 
the hero, consoled by his father, who has faith in him, eventually succeeds; 
the heroine of the second story meets with ridicule, becpmes discouraged and 
decides to leave her job. 

The following two stories from another protocol illustrate the effect of 
recognition on the work of the individual. 

This is a man and his wife celebrating. He has just received a promotion 
and he's telling her what happened when he got the promotion. What the 
manager said and he tells her what a good guy the manager is and how he 
said they were pleased with his work and he was promoted. She tells him to 
work hard and he says he'll kill himself working. He suggests to her that they 
go out and celebrate and they go out and celebrate. 

This is" a fellow who's drunk. When he was younger he was a very hard 
worker and never drank. He worked in one place years. He kept telling him- 
self the only way to get ahead was hard work. But after a while he began to 
think he was a fool. Nobody ever noticed how hard he worked, it was for 
nothing. The boss sat in his office and never noticed anything or anybody. He 
thought he was too good for the rest of them. There was no sense in anybody 
killing themselves for him. He took to having a little drink hi the afternoon 
and now you see what's happened to him. He's drunk and some guys are 
helping him home. 

In both of these stories the hero is a hard working man; in the first story 
the couple are going out to celebrate the hero's success in work, and he 
resolves to work even harder. In the second story however the hero has 
failed in his work and is being helped home because he is drunk. There is 
a difference in the hero's attitude toward authority. In the first story he tells 
his wife "what a good guy the manager is" but in the second story the boss is 
described as someone who "sat in his office and never noticed anything or 
anybody. He thought he was too good for the rest of them." The factor in 
these stories responsible for this difference is the recognition accorded the hero. 
In the first story he gets a promotion, a symbol of recognition and the manager 
tells him that "they were pleased with his work," whereas in the second story 
"nobody ever noticed how hard he worked, it was for nothing." This is in 
fact an individual who is a hard worker but one who continues to work hard 
only if his effort is rewarded by recognition. 

The next two stories reveal an individual's cathexis of stable, unchanging 
work conditions. 

This man, the younger one, is a clerk in an office and doesn't make much 
money but his job suits him. This day the head of the office calls him in to talk 
to him and asks him if he would like to try his hand at selling. The younger 
man. is rather startled by the idea, he likes what he's doing now and doesn't 



262 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

want to change. But he doesn't know what to say. He finally mumbles some- 
thing about wanting some time to think it over and leaves. He tries to think 
of some way to say no. He finally leaves and gets another job as a clerk the 
lesser o two evils. 

This woman is an old school teacher, she looks like one. She's been teaching 
the same class for years now and she loves it. She knows eVery brick in the 
school building. She's taught in the same old classroom. The only thing that 
ever changes is the pupils and even they don't change much, they look like 
their parents did before them. She loves teaching beloved subjects, which 
don't change through the years. 

The hero in both of these stories likes his work. In the first story, how- 
ever, he leaves his job and in the second he doesn't. He leaves in the first story 
because he is asked to change the work he is doing. But the heroine of the 
second story loves her work and continues doing it because it does not change 
"through the years." Change as such is negatively cathected. Any change in 
the stable conditions of the work environment may disrupt the adjustment of 
such an individual. 

The individual may, however, have no wish whatever to "work." In the 
case of one person seeking vocational guidance, the TAT protocol indicated 
that his interest lay primarily in impressing others. He asked the vocational 
counselor what type of work he "should" seek, confessing that he had no 
preferences. The following stories are representative of his present confusion. 

This fellow apparently feels held back by uncertainty. He doesn't look overly 
energetic but I suppose he's trying to shake the repressed or lethargic feeling off. 
He ought to make the effort and I suppose he will succeed when he makes up 
his mind in which direction to dodge. 

I could imagine a man who once, when younger, sat on a curbstone watching 
the fire engines whistle by, thinking of all the choices for careers he had before 
him. Now the man feels he has already unconsciously and indirectly made 
some choice, although he never did make up his mind but rather drifted along, 
trying several kinds of work but never clearly preferring any one or doing 
any one well. He thinks maybe I should have made up my mind then on the 
curbstone and stuck with it. He could do a lot of things, but diffidence and 
idleness confuse him and have taken away his momentum. He lacks drive, 
gets to be too submissive. Maybe he can start over again now. Or on the other 
hand maybe he is already too old to do some of those "from the bottom up" 
stories himself. So he will continue to drift, stay near the edge of his polite 
society and continue his job, as, say, file clerk because he never felt the urge to 
accomplish better things. 

Tough life for this fellow, no friends, nowhere to go. Or if he can go to some 
of his old haunts, His friends who have established themselves will turn up 
their noses to learn that he is still where they passed him five or ten years ago, 
not having accomplished as much. He is trying to make up a good story to 
tell them, to justify himself, before he starts off to anywhere where he might 



DIAGNOSIS OF PERSONALITY: WORK SETTING 263 

meet the old crowd of classmates. He is really in the fog. He will finally get 
up enough gumption to move and when he gets to the old haunts will probably 
find a lot of new faces anyway which partially solves the problem but he will 
still be concerned more with the "story" than with the performance. 

He is clearly more concerned with the "story** he is going to tell his 
friends than with his actual performance, but he "never felt the urge to accom- 
plish better things." Although he would like to impress others, there is no 
urge to do anything which might earn him such applause. 

As we might have supposed, those heroes who experience any enjoyment 
in what they are doing are indulging in exhibitionism. Although all heroes 
would like to be impressive there are only two heroes for whom this satis- 
faction is possible. These two stories follow. 

Maybe this is some kind of hypnosis demonstration. Easier to imagine myself 
doing the hypnotizing than being the patient Can't figure out why this 
should be going on. (In the old days, so I'm told, hypnosis used to work as a 
cure for certain ailments.) Maybe this fellow is demonstrating before a radio 
audience. Will make the kid do a couple of tricks while "under the influence" 
to impress the audience. 

This chap is having a grand time swinging on a rope in the gym. He likes 
to show off. Maybe he imagines he is a sailor. He is climbing to the top 
and asking if anyone else can match him. There are some other fellows who 
accept the challenge and try their mettle. 

When told by the counselor that his basic interest was exhibitionistic lie 
was somewhat disturbed but also relieved. "I have always known I was not 
really interested in any job for its own sake," he said. A week later, however, 
this individual was less certain that the counselor was right. As he expressed it, 
"there was something to what you've said" but he seemed to have reservations. 

For reasons which need not concern us here, this person was incapable of 
completely accepting advice either from his father or father surrogates. He had 
previously consulted a psychiatrist and another psychologist whose advice he 
had accepted only with many reservations. Such an exhibitionistic wish might 
of course have been gratified had he chosen to be an entertainer. Since this 
was not possible he was advised to choose from among the jobs open to him 
the one which would offer him the greatest rewards for his exhibitionism and 
demand the least expenditure of energy. Psychotherapy previously undertaken 
had not altered his basic personality sufficiently to warrant any expectation that 
his exhibitionistic need could be modified. Consequently the counselor chose 
to regard this need as the "given" in the situation, directing the individual into 
those areas where the need might be less frustrated than it had been for jnost 
of his life. 



264 THE, THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

SUMMARY 

We have attempted a delineation of dimensions pertinent to an analysis 
of the individual's adjustment in the vocational setting. Work, we said, may 
have a positive or negative meaning for the individual. Examples of some of 
the negative motivating forces were work to overcome inferiority, poverty, and 
financial insecurity, or to overcome the psychological effects of a physical handi- 
cap. On the positive side we found work to create beauty, to excite applause, 
or to become famous or great. 

The sequence of levels characteristic of the work of the individual was 
considered and the relationship between the levels of daydream and behavior 
was seen to vary between individuals. Daydreams, it was found, may have 
a tonic effect on some individuals, spurring them to work and sustaining them, 
whereas for others they provide a retreat from work or the individual finds 
them so exciting that his intention to work is short-circuited in the daydream. 
Phantasy may, however, constitute an important aspect of creative work. 

Phantasy, regardless of its content, may inhibit or excite work for some 
individuals while for others the effect is differential: certain types of day- 
dreams are disruptive to work while others are stimulating. 

Conceptual activity, we saw, was the typical forerunner of work for some 
individuals, whereas others turned to this level only after initial trial and error 
activity. There was not always further clarification or wcrk from such 
reflection. 

Differentiation of work into means and ends was discovered to depend on 
the nature of the work. Work which offered no proMems was not so dif- 
ferentiated. For the creation of such a distinction there had to be some 
discrepancy between the wish and the achievement. 

An examination of the degree of means-end cognizance appeared useful 
in assessing the realism of the individual. Too high a degree of such cogni- 
zance, however, might paralyze action. We distinguished means-end elabora- 
tion in daydreaming from its counterpart on the level of behavior. These 
were sometimes found to be different in the stories of the same individual. 

It was shown that means might be cathected only in so far as they con- 
tributed to some end or that such means might, through experience, come to be 
positively cathected for their own sake. Certain aspects of the total work 
process might be cathected, and one of these is the barrier which must be 
overcome before the end may be achieved. The conquest of barriers may in 
fact become an end in itself. 

The complexity and organization of work attributed to heroes was shown 
to be an index of the individual's actual capacities. 

We next examined the temporal characteristics of work. We differentiated 
rime perspective from complexity and organization of work. We found that 



DIAGNOSIS OF PERSONALITY: WORK SETTING 265 

an individual might possess a long time perspective, carefully planning his 
work over a long period of time but that the work might be relatively simple, 
lacking complexity of organization. We also differentiated the dimension of 
endurance, since the hero might have a long time perspective and yet work for 
only a short time. Finally, we examined changes in the ego ideal or in work 
habits as a function of time. The hero as a child, and the hero as an adult 
were seen, not infrequently, to work in different ways to achieve ends which 
were conspicuously disparate in character. 

We addressed ourselves next to the criteria used in the determination of 
the relative importance of the work region. Direct reference, number and 
length of stories concerning the work region and the affect and intensity of 
stories were shown to be useful in such assessment. Further criteria were the 
introduction of strivers, the interpretation of ambiguous figures as strivers, 
the restructuration of passive pictures as a preparation for activity, the omis- 
sion of passive figures, identification with active figures, and rejection of 
passivity. The relative importance of the work region may also be revealed 
by an examination of the degree of conflict between this and other regions, and 
the resolution of such conflict. Related is the criterion of the impact of work 
on other regions. We found evidence that there may be an ego ideal centered 
on work which structures the personality and enables the individual to tolerate 
frustration in other regions. 

Finally, we inquired into the conditions of work, illustrating that work 
for different individuals depended on such conditions as the stimulating char- 
acter of the work, a minimum of coercive dominance from the employer, the 
faith of others in the worker, recognition from the employer, unchanging 
conditions of work and sufficient reward for exhibitionistic wishes. These 
represented only a sample of the conditions which commonly determine the 
individual's adjustment in the vocational setting. 



CHAPTER X 
DIAGNOSIS AND PSYCHOTHERAPY 

A. DIAGNOSIS * 

use of the TAT as a diagnostic instrument is limited by the uncer- 
tainty of inferences based on this procedure. No less serious is the admitted 
inadequacy of present-day classification of mental diseases. These do not, in 
our opinion, represent homogeneous entities even at the level of symptom- 
atology. It is for these reasons that correlations between TAT results and 
these entities cannot be far reaching. Such effort would be better applied to 
the reclassification of mental disease on the basis of homogeneous test patterns. 

It is accepted dogma that one cannot altogether trust what the patient says 
of his own condition. Yet much of present-day psychiatric practise proceeds 
on the basis of evidence derived from the interview situation. The interview 
material one must indeed have, but there may be crucial gaps in the material 
necessary for diagnosis and therapy if some "depth" technique is not employed 
in addition to the interview. The TAT affords a relatively economical method 
of exploration before therapy is undertaken. 

Typically we find a contrast between what the patient is willing or able 
to tell and his covert or unconscious antisocial wishes. These are generally 
sexual or aggressive wishes which occasion the individual anxiety. But there 
are many instances in which the suppressed or repressed wishes are not anti- 
social and are more readily capable of "being assimilated into the adult per- 
sonality if they can be discovered and brought to the individual's consciousness. 

In one such case, mentioned previously, the individual, while in the 
armed forces, had worked in a laboratory under the direction of a medical 
scientist. Under the paternalistic and benevolent guidance of this man he had 
worked effectively. Then one day he was bitten by a dog. The entire year 
following this he lived with the intense fear that he might have contracted 
rabies. He had experienced anxiety attacks before he had come to the labora- 
tory but while there he had been free of such attacks. During the year fol- 
lowing his injury, however, there was a reappearance and exacerbation of his 
anxiety. Then he was discharged from the armed forces. When first seen he 
had renounced his plans for marriage and his medical career. He complained 
of a complete lack of self confidence and an inability to concentrate on his 
work. He was afraid to resume his medical studies because he could not work, 

* By diagnosis we mean the general assessment of any pathological condition rather than 
the determination of a specific nosological entity. 

266 



DIAGNOSIS AND PSYCHOTHERAPY 267 

afraid to marry lest he be an inadequate husband and his wife turn away from 
him if she were to appreciate the extent of his inadequacy. There was an 
important sense in which these fears were realistic. His anxiety was so intense 
that at crucial moments, while taking an examination, for example, his real 
ability was so reduced that he was indeed incompetent. Although the original 
anxiety may have been based on unreal expectations, the sequence of anxiety, 
paralysis, and reduction in competence provided a realistic basis for further 
anxiety. 

One would expect in such a case to find a TAT protocol weighted with 
anxiety and feelings of inadequacy. But this was not altogether the case. In 
striking contrast to his verbalized feelings an optimistic forward-looking picture 
emerges in much of his TAT record. In these stories there are defined the 
particular conditions under which this individual once did, and might again, 
function without fear. It is noteworthy that his "slips of the tongue" in 
interviews were in the same direction. He said on certain occasions "this set 
me up, I mean upset me very much." Let us examine the first story told by 
this individual. 

A picture of a young boy with a violin who seems to be sitting in complete 
awe upon reception of a new violin. He possibly was a child protege with a 
deep and complete understanding of music and his receiving of the violin has 
presented his opportunity to go on and develop his talent and great love into 
becoming one of the world's great artists. 

Looking into the past he pictures himself as a child protege who has been 
presented with the opportunity for development and future achievement. 
Notice that this was mediated through an unmentioned benefactor. In the 
next story we are told something of his adolescence. 

Here is a picture of a young man participating in a gym class. This young 
man was probably underdeveloped as a young child and consequently was 
subject for the taunts of his fellow youths for his inability to play or 
participate in the more strenuous games. With a feeling of inferiority he made 
up his mind that he would take every means at his disposal to develop himself. 
Entering into gym classes he worked hard and in time did develop the bulging 
muscles and the stamina that seemed so necessary to win the respect of his 
fellow youths. Here in this picture he is winning the rope climbing event and 
from the expression on his face, a great deal of satisfaction. 

This represents almost a literal page from his own youth. He did strive 
to overcome feelings of physical inferiority by taking boxing lessons and became 
a good amateur boxer. When he later joined the armed forces this was partly 
the basis on which he won the respect of his new friends. Notice that the 
conditions for success are here more specifically documented. This is accom- 
plished thro.ugh instruction and his own strenuous effort. In another story 
we see his work under the medical scientist. 



268 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

Here the impression gained is of an old man a man who appears to represent 
success, possibly a successful doctor who has trained the young man in the 
picture trying to pass on all the wisdom he has accumulated over long years. 
The time has come for the older man's retirement and he is having one of his 
last long talks with the young man he has selected and trained to be his successor 
who I would say does "carry on" as successfully. The picture could be entitled 
*The old master and his successor." 

This is a picture of the self confidence which was actually his while he 
worked for the older man in the medical laboratory. 

Tracing the changes from early childhood to this point, we see that his 
self confidence varied at different ages; as a child he was relatively self 
confident, as an adolescent, he had a feeling of inferiority which was overcome 
and as a young adult he had complete self confidence under special conditions. 

The conditions for his achievement have also undergone changes in time. 
In childhood we were told only that someone presented him with the oppor- 
tunity; in adolescence he received impersonal instruction in the "gym class.'* 
But in early adulthood the instruction is seen to be crucial, and this instruction 
has to be given paternalistically by a "successful" doctor whom he can respect. 
Inquiry revealed that he could work only for a distinguished scientist whom 
he respected and from whom he felt he could learn. For this man he would 
work late into the night in order to profit from his apprenticeship. He rejected 
other scientists in the laboratory who respected him for his knowledge of the 
more recent literature. In fact he turned down the offer of partnership in a 
medical practice with one of these men because "he wanted to learn from me 
and I couldn't learn anything from him," It is important to differentiate his 
need from that of passive dependency. His wish is that the older man teach 
him all he knows so that he will later te able to function adequately by himself. 
The interruption of this apprenticeship, his injury and his discharge from the 
armed forces exacerbated his anxiety. 

We are next presented with a picture of his present state. 

A young man, a soldier suffering from some neurotic disorder possibly a partial 
paralysis is receiving the benefits of physical neurotic medicine. Clinically it has 
been proven that the young man suffers from no organic disorder so he has 
been referred to the psychological clinic. Here the doctor through hypnotism 
is attempting to unlock from his mind the possible cause of the paralysis. The 
doctor is, of course, successful and the young man*s condition is "cleared up" 
or the paralysis goes. 

It is of interest that although he actually suffered a bite on the hand there 
is here mention of partial paralysis. Whether this symptom might have 
appeared had the anxiety level increased we cannot say. Notice that despite 
the neurotic disorder the soldier has faith in the older doctor who practices 



DIAGNOSIS AND PSYCHOTHERAPY 269 

the techniques of modern science. The doctor is "of course" successful. It 
was just this confidence which the patient verbally disclaimed. 

We see in another story that where the problem is less serious, he thinks 
his own resources are adequate. 

Here is a picture which could be entitled "Lonesomeness." This young man 
portrayed in the picture has severed the ties of home and gone of? to a 
strange section of the country to work. Having not previously been away from 
home he of course suffers many periods of lonesomeness. At times the feelings 
are so acute he is about to give up and return to home and familiar surround- 
ings. However, being a young man possessed of a lot of the good old-fashioned 
"stick-to-itiveness" he hangs on and with time meets people, develops comrade- 
ships with them while slowly but progressively losing his periods of lonesomeness. 

This individual was living at home and one of the reasons he hesitated 
either to resume his medical education or to marry was his avowed "fixation" 
to his family, and yet we see that he does possess confidence in his ability to 
cope with new situations and with "lonesomeness" another contrast to his 
verbalized feelings. 

When confronted with his own stories he disclaimed their relevance. He 
insisted that they were simply "stories" and that he could have made them 
much more pessimistic had he tried. A few days later however he volunteered 
the information that he felt better in the presence of the therapist. It was at 
this time that I pointed out to him that when he had worked for the medical 
scientist whom he respected he had also been free of anxiety. This he admitted 
somewhat reluctantly. I then pointed out that this was reflected in his stories 
his confidence was great when he was instructed or helped by someone who 
was expert he expected that he would "of course" be cured. He again 
resisted the interpretation and it was not until some weeks later that he himself 
saw these relationships and decided to act upon them to carry out his plans 
for marriage and to resume his medical studies, planning to work again under 
the personal guidance of a distinguished medical scientist. 

This individual had been "reassured" by others to no avail. He had 
resisted all attempts to tell him that he was "all right." Transference for him 
was based not on love but on respect, and more particularly in later years, 
respect for the man of science, the adult prototype of the omniscient parent. 
In this particular case it was of no little importance that the diagnosis rested 
on a "scientific" test. His primary phantasy involved learning the wisdom of 
the parent and then taking his place. This he hoped to do with the loving 
cooperation of the parent. But the parental surrogate today must be someone 
who stands in the same relation to the adult as the father did to the child. 



270 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

B. THERAPY 

I. ELICITING REPRESSED MEMORIES 

It is problematical whether the recovery of repressed memories is an 
agent of cure or whether it is simply the resultant of an increased tolerance 
for these memories achieved in some other way. Whatever their ultimate 
worth, memories long unavailable to consciousness may be elicited through 
use of the TAT. In the intensive study of Z reported previously, interviews 
by myself and others failed to elicit memories of material particularly prominent 
in the TAT. These interviews extended over the entire testing period. In 
this ten month period pointed questions revealed very few early memories and 
none pertinent to the TAT stories. After he had completed all the TAT 
stories, Z was confronted with the following sample of the stories he had told 
during the school year. 

At first, this picture reminded me of a fish but after looking at it a little 
while it appears to be a thumb with a drop of fluid on it. Perhaps this fluid 
is blood and this is a cue to the solution of a crime. All in all, the picture 
is striking and has an appearance of cold about it. 

The picture is one of a big toe. An anatomy student has cut it off a body 
and put it into the tux pocket of another fellow. At a formal dance this 
student took out his handkerchief and this toe fell out on the refreshment 
table. 

This thumb has been severely burned. It belonged to a criminal and skin grows 
back on with a different thumbprint. This causes havoc in the field of 
criminology and many theories are advanced as to the reason of this exception. 
The criminal later escapes and commits crimes that for a while baffle the police 
because of the prints. This, of course, is many years later. Before they catch 
him again he commits suicide. 

This man is an Indian fakir and he is climbing up a coil of rope that he has 
stiffened. Halfway up he sees a purple bird which is a bad omen. The rope 
become* Ump, he falls to the ground, breaking a leg. 

The upper woman had a pet dog she loved dearly. The other woman 
accidentally stepped on the dog's foot and caused it to whine. The mistress 
became infuriated and strangled the woman. 

There has just been a murder committed. The man has killed his sister and 
dazed and stunned stumbles out and stubs his toe. Gangrene sets in and his 
toe is cut off. Then he is hung. 

Another idea is that the baby lost its right hand and the possessor of the large 
hand is thinking how limited the growing boy will be. However, the baby 
will astound everyone with its one-handed achievements. 

Bats and owls. Man has become thoroughly soused. The bats pick all his 
hair out He is bald. He is very much ashamed of becoming drunk. He has 
a fixation for bats now because he is bald. 



DIAGNOSIS AND PSYCHOTHERAPY 27! 

Confidentially if I were that guy I'd get a hair cut. Me I need a hair cut 
too. Oh you with the power of life and death. I refuse sir. I writhe in 
torment. . . . And the orange fell in little pieces and the little pieces turned 
into shears and cut off his hair and one of them slipped and cut off his ear, 
and that's how I lost my left ear. 

The boy is an engineer and is wondering how the mechanics of a violin work. 
When he grows up he'll become a physicist and work on the problems of 
sound and acoustics. In the midst of the work an explosion deprives him of 
both hearing and sight. After a long recovery he starts to work again, never 
accomplishing much. 

This man is blind and he is thinking back over the days when he couldn't see 
and wishing something or ether. As he thinks, a river rushes by and the 
noise opens his eyes. He sees green cocktail shakers and then becomes blind 
again. 

Milton went blind. I'd be crushed by blindness. I couldn't see a damn 
thing. Have to live within myself and I'm a horrible being. 

This old lady has been having trouble with her eyes and so she has sent away 
to a mail order house to get a prescription. They seem to help her, but after 
a couple of weeks her eyes once again bother her and she goes to a doctor 
who examines her eyes and her glasses and tells her not to wear her glasses and 
to rest her eyes. Later she goes blind from a hereditary weakness. 

Similar phantasies were also prominent in his dreams: 

I was arguing with mother whether babies' umbilical cords should be stepped on 
or cut. I said that if they were cut the baby would get some terrible disease. 

After reading these stories Z expressed surprise at the degree of mutila- 
tion anxiety they indicated and thought they referred to "something" but 
didn't know what. Then suddenly there was a burst of memories which shed 
much light on the origin of these fears of castration and body mutilation. The 
most significant of these was the memory of the tune his mother caught his 
brother masturbating. He overheard the threat that she would "cut it off and 
put it in the soup" if he ever did it again. Another memory dates from the 
age of 8, when he caught the skin of his penis in a loose nut o.a fire hydrant 
where he had been playing. "The experience was painful, especially because 
there was a short period in which I didn't think I could get it free. I didn't 
tell anyone about it and I guess I fairly well repressed the memory but not the 
fear that went with it." He also remembered his mother saying that she 
didn't like to leave children with a nurse because nurses taught children to 
play with themselves. He remembers masturbating with both his left hand 
and left foot. His father ran over his foot with a motor car. He was not 
sure whether this was a fact or phantasy. It probably is phantasy. He says 
"my father running over my foot probably happened between the ages of 



272 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

4 and 7 and it didn't hurt." He was warned by his father not to masturbate 
"and it was with strong guilt feelings that I continued it. Although my 
religion was a weak one, I had fears that those among the dead who knew me 
were watching and condemning me. Daring this year I had my first shave. 
Curiously it was under threat from my father and brother who threatened me 
that they would shave me in bed. Most adolescents are only too proud to 
shave for the first time. I still dislike shaving. Freshman year at college 
I had a long siege of anxiety about losing my hair and becoming bald before 
I graduated. The external cause was dandruff, but the fear was great. I 
almost have a compulsion about entering haircuts in my diary. I tried to 
rationalize it, that it was to keep a record of how often I had a haircut. I came 
to college with 20-20 vision. That same fall I went home and because my 
eyes had been bothering me I saw a doctor who gave me glasses. According 
to test I was nearsighted. Two years later my eyes got worse. They are now 
20-70 and 20-50. Sometimes I feel horribly tense and when I go to sleep I 
can hardly relax my eyes. Recently I've been trying to relax them." Whether 
this decrease in visual acuity was the resultant of increased anxiety and tension 
we do not know. 

The recovery of these memories excited the subject, gave him insight into 
some of the sources of his anxiety and was a cathartic experience. So far as 
could be determined, however, the ultimate therapeutic gain was slight. 

2. ASSESSMENT OF ATTITUDES TOWARD THERAPY 

More important than the eliciting of repressed memories is the use of the 
TAT to determine the individual's attitude toward therapy and toward the 
therapist. While prognosis is not necessarily correlated with this attitude, 
a knowledge of it will be useful in deciding such questions as the degree of 
directiveness or nondirectiveness therapy should employ. It is of some impor- 
tance to know in advance exactly what the individual wants and expects from 
therapy and whether he gives any indication of willingness to change. 

Stories in which these attitudes are revealed may be told to any picture, 
but the picture of the younger man and the hypnotist (12) is of particular 
value in eliciting such material. 

The following story gives at once the patient's attitude toward his own 
problem and his attitude toward the therapist. 

Hypnotist hypnotizing some young man trying to mesmerize him--no he's 
fast asleep he'll awaken sooner or later trying to solve his problem that way 
Don't know the outcome think he'll be helped to solve his problem. 

He is uncertain about the therapeutic outcome and equally uncertain 
whether it is he or the therapist who will be able to help him. This person 



DIAGNOSIS AND PSYCHOTHERAPY 273 

suffered from a pathological need to sleep; he was hardly able to keep awake 
during the day. This story tells us that sleeping is not only a defense but an 
attempt to solve his problem. 

In the following case, we see a definite rejection of the therapist. 

This boy has been studying very very hard been reading a lot, taken many 
examinations and the mother was worried. She finally decided to send him to 
a psychiatrist. Here the psychiatrist is putting the boy at ease, perhaps even 
putting him under the influence of hypnosis. The boy is a brilliant one. He 
has not been overworking or overstudying although he has been doing quite 
a bit of each or both. His mother feels that there may be something in his 
mind that he cannot bring forth hence the psychiatrist. The boy is put under 
an hypnotic spell and is made to talk by the psychiatrist, talks of very many 
things, but one significant thing to the psychiatrist is seems to be a key to 
the solution of this boy's trouble. He talks of his fear of the water of how 
he had been pushed into the river by older boys in an effort to make him 
swim when he was just a youngster. He talks about this for quite a while 
and then stops entirely. The doctor lets him sleep for about one-quarter of an 
hour and then induces him to awaken. He talks with the boy about various 
things, his activities at school or play, recreation, feelings toward girls takes 
notes as he goes along. The boy seems to be a willing patient. The questioning 
goes on for about ten minutes longer. He thanks the boy and tells him to go 
home. In a day or two he has a report for his mother. The woman cornes 
to his office. He gives her what seems to her long and detailed explanations of 
her boy's condition. When she asks the doctor to boil it down to simple English 
it comes down to the simple fact that the doctor sees nothing really wrong 
with the boy. He is a normal boy who has gone through a nervous stage and 
will snap out of it suddenly. The mother seems to be rather disappointed. It 
couldn't be as simple as that, she feels, and after having spent so much money 
in consultation with this man she is a little bewildered. Thanks the doctor 
and walks out of his office thinking to herself that all these psychiatrists arc 
fakers. 

This was an individual who was, in fact, willing to travel a few hundred 
miles for vocational guidance and who said he was very much impressed with 
modern diagnostic methods. But this story would indicate that he covertly 
resented the money involved and felt that the scientific aspect of the whole 
procedure was both pretentious and trivial. 

Z, who it will be remembered suffered excessive castration anxiety a told 
the following story to the hypnotist picture. 

The man lying down is in a coma. He is suffering from a painful disease and 
is in the last stages. The other man is a doctor. He realizes there is no cure 
for the sick man who is a close friend of his and as he sits there he places a 
hand on his forehead and offers a prayer that his friend's pain may soon be over. 

The therapist's intentions are good. He is a "close friend," but the 
malady from which the patient suffers is beyond help. 



274 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

In the following case there was a more realistic expectation of the nature 
of the therapeutic relationship. 

Obviously a psychologist who is using the mode of hypnosis to relieve the 
tension of the subject's mind. Subject being a boy of 17, lying flat on a 
couch. The psychologist has just completed the seance and is about to bring 
the boy out of it. Boy has interesting story. He has come to the psychologist 
because he had conflict between his own desires and those of his family con- 
cerning his future. His family wished him to become a pillar of the church 
and perfectly acceptable normal man. When he had gone to college he had 
discovered much more enjoyment in life besides the grim austerity of a 
puritanical background. Being a loyal son, he finds it difficult to reconcile 
what he wants to do with what his family wants him to do. The psychologist 
has not been entirely successful because tension is still evident on boy's face. 
It will take more than one seance to bring the boy to a successful appraisal of 
his difficulty. He will realize that he has his own life to live, own responsibilities 
to himself, and family will become less and less important. 

This individual expected help but realized this could not be effected in 
one "seance" and that he would have to assume some of the responsibility for 
his cure. 

In another case this picture did not elicit such phantasies, since the subject 
identified with the hypnotist who loved to show off before an audience. But 
in a story told to picture 7 (a picture of an older man and a younger man) we 
find a portrayal of his attitude toward paternal authority, which reflected the 
attitude transferred to the therapeutic situation. 

This might be a father and son scene. (A) Maybe they are hatching plans for 
a vacation together or (B) maybe the father is giving advice to the son. 
Father has slightly more pleasant expression than the son. If (A), they will 
go off together and have a good time, though maybe they aren't too well 
acquainted, if (B) son will accept advice, but with private reservations that 
maybe father doesn't understand. 

As vocational counselor I wondered whether he would show the same 
"private reservations" about my advice. This seemed likely, inasmuch as he 
felt the psychiatric treatment he had received previously was of doubtful value 
and he had been to a university vocational counseling service which he also 
felt had not helped him. When I revealed to him his need for recognition 
and his exhibitionism, he was somewhat upset but also relieved, admitting that 
he had always known that he was not really interested in any job for its own 
sake. When I saw him a week later, it was clear that his "private reservations" 
had nullified much of the insight he had achieved. In his words "there was 
something to what you've said" i.e., partial acceptance as it was delineated ir 
the father-son story but with "private reservations.'* 



DIAGNOSIS AND PSYCHOTHERAPY 275 

We reproduce again, for the sake of contrast, the story of the individual 
whose anxiety state was precipitated by the bite of a dog. 

A young man, a soldier suffering from some neurotic disorder possibly a partial 
paralysis is receiving the benefits of physical neurotic medicine. Clinically it 
has been proven that the young man suffers from no organic disorder so he 
has been referred to the psychological clinic. Here the doctor through 
hypnotism is attempting to unlock from his mind the possible cause of the 
paralysis. The doctor is, of course, successful and the young man's condition 
is "cleared up" or the paralysis goes. 

TAT as Play Therapy 

The TAT is also useful, in some cases, as a type of play therapy. It must 
be admitted that the conditions under which it may and may not be useful 
in this connection are still obscure. We shall present in some detail a case in 
which the TAT was used by the writer as a diagnostic instrument but in which 
it served a useful therapeutic purpose although this was not intended. 

This child aged 6 suffered a variety of phobias and a generalized chronic 
anxiety. He was afraid of running water and would grow panicky if anyone 
ran water into a basin or tub which was plugged, begging to have the plug 
released. When he flushed the toilet he would run away. These fears appeared 
to have their origin in an experience where water overflowing the bathtub 
spread through the house. His parents were away at the time. He also 
feared noises in general and the noise of the oil burner in particular and refused 
to go near the cellar when it was on. He is afraid of strange places and 
strange people. When he visits the houses of strangers with his parents he 
sometimes prefers to remain alone in the car for several hours rather than 
go in with them. He will no longer play with his former friends because 
they now play around the corner from his house, on a strange street. He 
refuses to go to the neighbcfrhood movies with his brother, who is three years 
older, or even with his mother. He cannot easily be left alone by his mother. 
On the one occasion when she was out of town for a day, she returned to find 
him in a severe anxiety state. After this he refused to go to school. There 
is general anxiety about going to school and he frequently vomits his breakfast 
on the school bus. When his brother joined the school orchestra club and 
couldn't accompany him home from school one afternoon a week, he refused 
to go to school. On these mornings he would wake at five o'clock and cry 
until it was time to go to school. He refuses to eat very much. It makes no 
difference whether his parents cajole, threaten, or ignore him. He is possessed 
of low frustration tolerance, frequently whining and crying and refusing to 
participate if he can't have his way. He also appears to have marked feelings 
of inferiority and when" encouraged to show his skill complains that "he can't 



276 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

do it" and will generally make no attempt. If he does try, his interest is 
short-lived; he lacks perseverance and will often fly into a rage at his own 
lack of skill or give the task to someone else to complete. 

When the child was three his father went off to the army and saw him 
only infrequently in a three year period. During this time he stayed with 
his mother and brother. His mother, having suffered an excessive amount of 
maternal dominance in her own childhood, leaned over backward in treating 
her own children dispassionately. 

It was in this context and in light of the age and distractibility o the child 
that the writer undertook to administer the TAT as a game. Both he and 
the child were to tell stories. This appealed very much to the child and we 
went off to another part of the house. He regarded it as a secret shared only 
by the examiner and himself, refusing to allow his older brother to be present 
at the session or to play the game until he had completely finished. The 
testing was distributed over a week, and the child constandy asked the examiner 
if they could play the "game." The child was also encouraged to participate 
in the stories told by the examiner, who pretended not to know what to say 
and allowed the child to demonstrate his superiority. The examiner treated 
in his own stories those themes which he knew were sources of anxiety to the 
child. The stories which follow are those told by the child himself. 

The first two stories of the protocol tell us little. They may show either 
inertia in beginning or resistance to revealing his phantasies. 

Boy writing looks like and he's looking at something I don't know what this 
thing is here I don't know what this thing is he's looking at the violin 
there's some paper underneath the violin the boy was probably playing the 
violin and then he put it down on a piece of paper and then (now I'm up to 
the other thing) then he didn't play any more and he put it down on the 
piece of paper think that's all I think. 

There's a lady she's holding her books in her hands this looks hard. And 
there's a horse there and I don't know what this is [E answers] that's a field 
and the horses are going on it and there's a lady over watching the horse 
looking at it. There's a house, a few barns, mountains and brushes and stuff 
and trees and everything looks like that's all. 

In the third story we see the beginning of the recital of his fears. 

Is that a man or a boy or a girl or what She's laying on a sewer pipe and 
she's sitting on the dirty floor where the rats are and there's a mouse coming 
and she ran away and she runs upstairs cause she's afraid of the mice and the 
rats and everything so the exterminators come and squirt (water) bugs all 
over the cellar then then the exterminators come again and squirt water all 
over the bugs and then they go home then the girl came down again and she 
found water all over and the exterminators came again and then she saw the 
pipe broken and then they called the plumbers, and the plumbers couldn't do 



DIAGNOSIS AND PSYCHOTHERAPY 277 

it and the pipes kept leaking and water got all over and then they didn't have 
any water left so they had to move to another house and the new pipes were 
so high that it wouldn't fit into the house they broke the other house and 
took some of the stuff they needed like a door and they used it like to 
make a darkroom. So when the girl went down that cellar there was a 
coal gas explosion then she went upstairs and told her mother and then when 
she came down the next day she saw an oil burner and she was so glad. And 
then the next day the oil burner went on she got so scared that she ran 
upstair crying so after that she got so scared that she got happy again and 
she didn't know it was Christmas and she got so many toys she asked her 
mother if it was Christmas and her mother said yes "it was" so when she 
heard that it was Christmas She got so glad that she got too many toys to 
play with (she didn't have time to use all those) she put some away in the 
cellar so next Christinas she still had those and she didn't know it (she 
forgot) after she found them (next Christmas) she still had those toys from the 
last Christmas to the third Christmas (she didn't know) but after she found 
out that her mother buys toys before Christmas. She looked for them and she 
found them then after she found them she didn't let her mother see them she 
saw that they were swept away from the diathermy with a broom so she looked 
in her son's room and she found those toys and she didn't give it to her son 
till the fourth Christmas and that's all. 

In story 4 we see the introduction of the magician, who i to play a very 
important part in these stories. He is later seen as the agent of therapy. 

Once upon a time there was a girl and a man and they were cousins so once 
there was a baby born and after they got that baby they got married and after 
the baby got big he came to be a magician -and he was the best magician in 
the whole world know who he was he was Houdini and after Houdini was 
big enough he got so big that he died and after that the next day he was 
buried and after he was buried he wasn't buried any more he jumped out 
because he was a magician and he died so he jumped out because he was a 
magician and after he died (funny stuff) he went up to Heaven and came 
down went up again and came down and after he came down from heaven 
he made everybody in the whole world disappear even God (God would 
never disappear would he? or Jesus?) and after that I'm finished this story 
and I'll start some more. One day a little boy came to the magician's house 
and the magician gave him some stuff and one of them was a tin soldier and 
that was Blackstone the magician and after that the mother and father dies 
and this boy was a man and he got married. (Who?) He married Mrs. 
Harnintush and after Hamintush died he died and there was nobody to 
marry and after that everybody else lived happily together. Only two what 
was living that was God and Jesus. After that twenty funny came in he was 
a new baby he got so big he got married and after he got married he died. 
And after he died there's mister Eighty skillion million billion he got 
married to the other one they both died together with their eyes open and 
looking at each other and got buried and jumped out of the hole that was 
funny. 



278 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

This story shows a preoccupation with thoughts of death and dying. 
There is little in the past history to give us a clue to the origin of these 
phantasies. When asked why people die he said, "it's because they eat the 
wrong food." When asked what the wrong foods are he gave them as germs, 
clorox, and ammonia. There is a strong possibility that this phantasy is closely 
linked with his refusal to eat, either because he believes that food is poisonous 
or because he believes that food might cause him to grow too big and he would 
then die. But there is no definite proof of either of these hypotheses and no 
explanation of the association between death and marriage which appears in 
these stones. 

We do, however, see that the magician is a kind of superman- who has 
the power to return from death and even from Heaven at will and that he 
possesses the power to make God disappear, although the child seems a little 
doubtful that either God or Jesus would disappear. 

The first three stories were told in the first session, and the fourth in the 
second session. In the third session we begin to see evidence of some thera- 
peutic gain. 

This woman was afraid of water and she couldn't drink water and when she 
got thirsty all of a sudden she got brave and she took a drink of water. It 
didn't hurt her. One day she got over being afraid. Maybe she was a baby 
and then she was a big girl. 

This change, as revealed by the TAT was accompanied by behavioral 
evidence reported by his mother. He undertook to clean the sidewalk of 
snow and remained at the job despite the cold until he had finished, in marked 
contrast to his usual abortive effort. Moreover, he offered, despite some 
anxiety, to go to the movies with his brother. 

In the final session, however, he is less concerned with overcoming his 
fears and gives his first happy ending, which endures ever after. 

After they got married they got some children and a magician came to their 
house, just when the children were 5 years old. The magician showed them 
some tricks. After the magician left they did some of the tricks. Then they 
had a broken pipe and a hole in the cellar floor the exterminators and plumbers 
came went away, and the children lived happily ever again* 

The magician has taught the child magic which enables him to live 
"happily ever again," despite the broken pipe. This therapeutic gain resulted 
in considerable diminution of his general anxiety level. His report at school 
changed. He had previously been something of a problem to his teacher 
because of his lack of confidence. An improvement has been reported in his 
adjustment in school and he is described as much more confident. He no 
longer vomits his breakfast on the school bus and is willing to go to school. 



DIAGNOSIS AND PSYCHOTHERAPY 279 

His fear of running water has abated and he flushes the toilet without running 
away. When the examiner saw him a month after the testing the child mani- 
fested an intense interest in magic and tricks. He demonstrated an experiment 
he had done in school, calling it a trick, appeared boastful in exhibiting his skill 
and insisted that the examiner watch him. 

There is still much to be desired in the reduction of this child's anxiety, 
but we are certain that the opportunity to express his fears through the medium 
of TAT stories in the permissive atmosphere this game provided was of thera- 
peutic efficacy. 

3. USE AS AN INSTRUMENT FOR CATHARSIS 

The TAT may further be useful in inducing catharsis of feelings which 
were not adequately expressed in a traumatic situation and were isolated 
because they threatened to overwhelm the individual. The TAT may be 
particularly effective in therapy of the repressed grief reaction. The indi- 
vidual in this case is aware of the death or loss of the loved object, suffers 
apathy or depression but refuses to renounce the object, and is hence incapable 
of developing new love relationships. 

The experience of telling TAT stories may be of therapeutic gain with 
children. But with adults it is generally more effective to give an interpreta- 
tion and then read to the patient the stories on which the interpretation is 
based. This was the procedure employed in the following case. 

This individual had suffered much insecurity throughout his life. He 
was first sent away by his mother following her divorce from his father. After 
sixteen years of marriage his wife deserted him. Two relationships followed 
which ended as unhappily. After the final rejection, there was a sharp but brief 
grief reaction which was consciously suppressed and never allowed full expres- 
sion. Instead he preserved objects which he had shared with his beloved 
souvenirs, dolls, curios, and phonograph records. A variety of experiences 
painful in memory going to the ballet, drinking tea, etc. were avoided. He 
experienced severe 'depression and apathy followed by loss of interest in love 
and sexual relationships. He transferred his remaining affect and energy to 
his work, overworking to keep from thinking of his grief. He was aware that 
he could not tolerate another rejection but was unaware of the depth of longing 
and grief still suppressed and isolated. 

The TAT proved to be most valuable in unlocking this affect. After a 
preliminary interpretation telling the individual that he was still suffering from 
this experience, the examiner read to him a few of the stories which revealed 
his suppressed longings. 

This boy or girl (boy probably) looks sad perhaps past crying at first you 
might think he fell, but the position does not bear that out, so he is crouching. 



280 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

Even if the objects on the floor to the left be some broken toy or more valuable 
article that should be handled carefully, the grief could not be caused by so 
trivial a matter. This is despair. It looks as though he had been out playing 
and had suddenly heard some devastating news the death of someone perhaps 
and he came in from play and was going to go up to his room, when it 
hardly seemed worth the effort to go up the steps after all, his room would 
be no different from here from anywhere especially if he has lost someone so 
dear so the world blows up in his face, and he collapses at the foot of the 
steps. How long will he be there? Only ten or fifteen minutes. By then he 
will have gotten hold of himself and will go up to his room. Things will 
look different in the room. Nothing will have much meaning. What brought 
him joy an aquarium, a toy boat he made now looks so useless so meaning- 
less. To him the room is empty -the world is empty. What to do? Yes 
what to do? Why of course, one does what one is supposed to dp. Whatever 
it is that one would have done before the bad news. Now what would that 
have been? Nothing has happened. And he won't want people to talk to him 
about what has happened to him. After all, who can understand who can 
care really so why listen to the drivel they spill on him with such righteous 
satisfaction that they are doing him a favor so for a while he will go about 
his business as usual, and gradually he will come out of it get over it 
apparently, and perhaps even forget it, apparently. And he will be a httle more 
patient and understanding with people who have suffered a similar loss but 
he will not sympathize with them to weaken them, rather he will show them 
how to grin and bear it the only way to overcome anything. And all other 
things being equal he can advance through life ready to absorb still more 
still greater disappointments so that what he is suffering now will seem 
trivial but this may be his first great hurt so it will stay with him forever. 

This projection into the past envisioned a future of disappointment, which 
he would "grin and bear." In stories concerned with the contemporary situa- 
tion, there is a different technique of dealing with the same problem. 

What do I see in this blank card? Helpless men if alone; happy, productive 
men if cooperative. So at first they wander about alone, each striving for his 
own existence, even fighting each other if threatened or even if the possibility 
of a threat appears. But as they kill each other off they stop to realize that 
there must be a way out of the fighting, if only all will look for it. And so 
science helps, whether to raise more food to feed more mouths, or to control 
births if there is not enough food, and then I see these men entering into 
peaceful pursuits with peaceful minds and hearts, not quite including the 
preachers though. But with these supplanted by sociologists, psychologists, 
experts in every area of peaceful purposeful productive living. So the scene has 
changed from fighting to helpfulness, from mere helpfulness to abundant 
productivity and consumption, with no solitude of mind, spirit, or body, 
but communion of all three with their kind without restraint. And the 
inevitable is accepted without question or quarrel is prepared for, is antici- 
pated, so that the pain that accompanies death and its IT" nor forerunners 
will be minimized both in the sufferer and in his loved ones, not through 
will power or great fortitude, but through proper concepts, proper condi- 



DIAGNOSIS AND PSYCHOTHERAPY 281 

tioning, proper perspective, and so full a life for all of us that none of us 
is too important to any of us yet where bonds should be close they can be 
much closer than now and yet the pain accompanying the dissolution of the 
bond should be less there should linger only a pleasant glow because of the 
richness of the good experiences that accompanied the forming of that bond. 
And the picture changes to the ultimate catastrophe of the world it is blotted 
out by a comet and all is for naught. 

Here is the same preoccupation, but it is now through "proper concepts," 
rather than "will power," that it is "hoped" the problem may be solved. This 
is clearly a new wish and a hope, but there is no certain conviction that the 
problem is soluble. 

In the next story the patient gives us a more accurate diagnosis. 

If this is sorrow phewy. The man should be sorry in his home who wants 
to stand by a grave? Whomever he has lost was not appreciated as an experi- 
ence which can not pass so long as memory persists. The carcass he stands 
over is nothing. So his sorrow is wasted. And his prayers too are wasted. 
And the whole concept of a life hereafter is not to be dwelt upon. We should 
not delude ourselves with any feeling of continuing consciousness, and if there 
is no consciousness on the part of the departed one, why worry about them 
worry about the one left so my hearty, go home about your business, and 
let the dead past bury its dead. If you were not smart enough to build into 
your life anything other than what is under your feet now, it is a little late, 
but you'd better do so at once. But he looks like such a dope that he may 
not take my advice and may mope and mope and mope. But if he has any of 
this world's goods (and is not mentally deranged too much anyway) some 
woman will undertake to console him as a means to a more complete life (not 
to say a more leisurely life) of her own always hoping of course that he too 
will get something out of the new arrangement which he should be able to 
if he will indeed bury his dead. So he will ultimately marry for convenience 
in a nice way not too cold-bloodedly. Now if this is his daughter lien he 
certainly is wasting his time. 

Here we see a realization that something was lacking in the past rela- 
tionship and that he cannot enter into a new relationship unless "he will indeed 
bury his dead." He is not certain that he can do this, and the outcome is 
therefore conditional. 

In his last story (picture 20) we are told why he hesitates to attempt a 
new love relationship. 

Looks like a giant caterpillar from upper left and about to devour the man. 
He seems indifferent to life not just out for a stroll. Yet not despondent 
He's hardly even wondering what to do next He'll 'just wait until the spirit 
moves him, so to speak; then he'll decide what to do. Meanwhile he is just 
standing there leaning up against the lamppost Perhaps he feels he should 
be doing something else, but what special difference does it make? So many 
things not done "on time" turn out not to be necessary after all. Important 



282 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

things usually have a way of making themselves known and are taken care of 
so he is just letting time pass. He probably has finished his day's work, had 
his dinner, and doesn't want to go to bed, yet has no place to go. No wonder 
he is dejected at least slightly. It would have to be a very special sort of 
person man or woman (but no children please) that might be welcomed by 
him for a brief chat. Who wants to hear most people's opinions on the elec- 
tions or styles or strikes or atom bombs or the next war? So he is a bit 
lonely especially since, should that special person come along, he or she 
would soon have to keep' on going about his or her business as he would 
have to too. So it is almost preferred not to meet that interesting person if 
the contact is to be so short. Yet, no matter how wise one's own thoughts, 
they are not enough, so he may settle for the stupidities heard around a bar 
or a dancehall or anywhere but alone in the park. Yet I wonder if he will 
get up enough desire to seek out suitable companions, he seems too listless. 
So Pll have him go to a bar, have a few drinks, be bored, and go home 
disgusted next night he will try another outlet always the same so he 
setties for work and even if that gets boring eventually, at least is an intellec- 
tual boring, not a stupid boring, and he feels the difference, much as he 
might profit by a good belly laugh and much as he may wish for such an 
environment, his duty, his work, or his morbid memories or something just 
seem to keep him from being able to get himself into that desirable environment. 
Yet I think he'll persist and eventually have the good luck to find a partner 
who seeking the same thing can find it in htm as he just found what he 
sought in the other. 

We are told that "it is almost to be preferred not to meet that interesting 
person if the contact is to be so short." So he buries himself in work, his 
"morbid memories" blocking any solution to the problem. And yet he thinks 
that eventually he may meet the right love object. 

Reading these stories to the patient was sufficient to release the affect which 
had been isolated from his memories of the mourned love object. The patient 
was shaken with grief. The catharsis afforded by this experience shattered 
the apathy which he had suffered since the disruption of his love relationship. 
This brief abreaction was followed by a further massive grief reaction, in which 
the original affect attending his loss was allowed full expression. Following 
this there was a slow regeneration of his interest in another love object. 

4. DIRECTIVE THERAPY 

In the case of individuals who are either too old or too inflexible or wher- 
ever intensive psychotherapy is impossible or inadvisable the TAT may be 
useful as a guide to directive therapy. 

As an example let us consider the case of a man in his middle fifties whose 
symptom was a curious inability to make correct change. When given a five 
dollar bill on his route, he would return change as though it had been a ten 
dollar bill, feeling compelled to do this. Under questioning it was revealed 



DIAGNOSIS AND PSYCHOTHERAPY 283 

that this behavior was particularly compulsive when he experienced most 
serious sexual frustration and with women who were sexually attractive to him. 
He suffered continual sexual frustration. For weeks after each sexual 
experience he felt debilitated. He was unable to keep awake, and experienced 
pain in his genitals which he described as a "raging toothache down there." 
He believed that intercourse was so harmful that he typically set the alarm 
clock to ring five minutes after he began intercourse with his wife and when 
it rang would terminate the intercourse. His wife had been suffering this 
frustration with him for eighteen years. He also believed that any position 
but the common one of man astride woman was perverted and might lead 
to even more serious consequences. Because of the reference to "a toothache 
down there" I asked him whether he ever bit his wife during intercourse. 
This he vehemently denied. Before the TAT was administered the examiner 
hypothecated a fusion of aggression with the sexual wish that he was in per- 
forming the sexual act venting aggression and that his pain and debility were 
punishment for what he had done. This was supported by many of his TAT 
stories, particularly by his interpretation of picture 13 depicting a naked woman 
in bed with a man standing over her. 

Husband has just killed or choked his wife quarrel of some kind bowed 
down with remorse he is greatly worried and doesn't know what to do. She's 
lying half undressed on the bed. The result will be that he will be punished 
for his acts act I mean. 

This individual had a long history of unhappy relationships with women. 
His mother left him when their home was broken. His first wife left him 
during the depression when he lost his job and though he has lived for many 
years with his second wife there appears to be a great deal of residual aggres- 
sion which he can express only in the act of sexual intercourse. 

It was in this context that the therapist adopted strictly directive therapy, 
trying to relieve the sex act of some of its symbolic aggressive meaning. The 
results of the TAT were not discussed at all but reassurance was given that sex 
was not harmful, that he could indulge in it freely and that he might adopt 
any position which he and his wife found satisfying. He was so electrified by 
this revelation that he asked the therapist to repeat it over and over again 
for an hour and a half. It is not difficult to understand why he could not 
believe this. It was as though the therapist had given him permission to 
murder his wife. Previously he had attended a Y.M.CA. course on sex in 
marriage and had left the course after hearing the lecturer advise that in 
marriage any position in sex was morally justifiable. Since this individual had 
a history of some 25 years of organic and psychiatric treatment we are certain 
that this was not the first time he had been reassured and that his acceptance 



284 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

of the therapist's suggestions might be short-lived. This we cannot say. He 
was never seen again, 

5. LIMITATIONS 

Useful as the TAT may be in diagnosis and therapy, there are many real 
limitations of its efficacy. In acutely disturbed individuals, the TAT may so 
intensify anxiety that it is contraindicated. In our experience, it may actually 
precipitate acute attacks of anxiety or depression. We do not yet know the 
conditions under which this will or will not happen. 

Further, when the person is acutely disturbed we frequendy find in the 
TAT stories nothing more than a reflection of this state with no indication 
whatever of the dynamics or origin of the state or even the content of anxiety. 
In certain cases, notably hysteria and anxiety, we find critical areas producing 
only shock reactions so that we can only say that there is anxiety but have 
no basis for further inference. Thus a typical hysteric reaction to a sex picture 
is that of the patient who says, "Gee, I don't know. . . . Kind of disgusting 
picture. ... I don't know. I suppose everything will be okay." 

Finally, it must not be supposed that one has only to read a patient his 
own phantasies and expect him to be capable of accepting and assimilating them. 

In the next case the patient was asked to free-associate to his own story. 

This girl has two books in her hands and she has just been turning in the 
opposite direction where she saw the woman leaning against the tree carrying a 
child pregnant and as she thought of the scene of domestic peace the 
woman carrying the child and the man, caring for the garden she said to 
herself "that is not for me yet Fll go back to my studies and later on I will 
give birth to a child or I will have a child." What brought her up to the 
scene is that she is interested in gardening and loved horses. The outcome of 
it is that she goes back to school and continues her studies. 

His associations to this story were as follows: 

I have always loved understanding which I have associated with my mother. I 
have always wanted to produce something. I have been ashamed of what 
I have produced particularly my master's thesis which was said to be the 
best one that had ever been done but it didn't satisfy me. As a boy I used 
to read the classics, used to daydream wondering whether I would be able to 
fulfil my ideals and aspirations. I set them very high higher than I would 
ever attain. Since then I know that ideals have a lot of the content of illusion 
about them. Sometimes I feel as though it isn't worth trying because I can 
never really attain what I am aiming for. My flight; into literature I think 
was an escape from reality. 

When it was pointed out that his wish to produce something and his 
shame at his efforts might be related to the story of the girl whose aim it was 
to "give birth to a child" or "have" a child, this was considered too fantastic 



DIAGNOSIS AND PSYCHOTHERAPY 285 

and bizarre a suggestion to be taken seriously. However, the frustration of 
his intense wish to create "something beautiful," a derivative of this earlier 
wish, was freely admitted. In this particular case therapy would have pro- 
ceeded at a much more rapid pace had the origins of his wishes been left 
undisclosed. He was very excited by modern architecture and wished to do 
creative design in this field. Had the therapist been able to "direct him into 
this without associating it with the identification with his mother, the sugges- 
tion would probably have been eagerly accepted. But the therapist's premature 
association of the present interest in creative design with the consciously 
unacceptable wish to create a child, . provoked anxiety about an ambition 
which had previously been completely satisfying. 

In this case the derivative of the original wish is freely admitted, the 
source denied. The reverse may also be true. The source is admitted, but 
the derivative is unacceptable. The following patient was also asked to free- 
associate to his own story. 

Here the impression gained is one of an old man who had early in his 
life done something which for the sake of a story say he had in some way 
"wronged" his wife. Possibly his wife had wanted children from the marriage 
and this man because of a dislike for children prevented him from carrying out 
his side of the marriage contract, that of having children. Then his wife in 
complete unhappiness had died. As the man advanced in years he, because 
of loneliness had become increasingly aware of just how cruel he had been to 
his wife and how through his cruelty to her had created his own state of 
unhappiness. Lonely and with sorrow in his heart he made his nightly 
pilgrimages to the grave of his wife where he prayed for forgiveness for what 
he had brought about. These pilgrimages he had maintained until his death. 

His free associations to this story were the following: 

It's a funny thing which I don't understand the other day my mother asked 
me to change rooms with my younger brother and his wife, who had come to 
live with us till they can find a place of their own. She said my room was 
larger and that they ought to have it. For some reason I suddenly saw red, 
and became unreasonably enraged, and though I didn't say anything I felt as 
though I would lose control of myself. I don't understand it 

Further questioning revealed a long history of fighting with his younger 
brother and hatred of his mother for the preferential treatment accorded his 
brother. In early childhood, he had frequently accused her of being unfair. 
But these differences had been reconciled as he grew older. This was the first 
outburst of his former hostility, and the patient was able to understand the 
relationship when it was shown to him. But when I suggested the possible 
consequences of this hatred of the sibling for his marriage and his own future 
children, it created anxiety and he vehemently denied this. He then denied 
that he had ever felt hostile towards either his brother or mother. During 



286 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

the next few days he experienced acute anxiety as the result of this inter- 
pretation. Within a week he was able to admit the intensity of his feelings 
towards both mother and brother, but he was not able to assimilate the possi- 
bility that this same feeling might be transferred to his wife and children if he 
married. It was only some weeks later that he asked what to do if he should 
find himself reacting in this way in the marital situation. 

In these two cases we find contrasting relationships between early and 
contemporary conflicts. In the first, the early wish is completely unacceptable, 
the derivative completely satisfying. In the second, the early hostility is known 
and admitted, the later consequences cannot be so easily tolerated. 

But there are numerous cases in which neither the source nor present 
derivatives revealed in the TAT can be assimilated into consciousness. The 
same assessment of the readiness of an individual to accept anxiety-laden 
material must be employed in the use of the TAT as in any other type of 
therapeutic effort. As with any effective instrument its use is attended with 
equal potentialities of harm and benefit. 

SUMMARY 

We have examined briefly the usefulness of the TAT as a diagnostic 
instrument in clinical practice, how it may be employed as a method of explora- 
tion before therapy is undertaken and how it may supplement interview 
material. 

As an adjunct of therapy the TAT may be helpful in eliciting memories 
long repressed. It may be used with more assurance in assessing the attitude 
of the patient toward psychotherapy. In the psychotherapy of children the 
TAT may be useful as a type of play therapy, although the conditions under 
which it may or may not serve such a function are still obscure. It has been 
found effective in stimulating catharsis of repressed feelings of grief, if inter- 
pretation is given, followed by reading to the patient those stories on which 
the interpretation is based. Whenever intensive psychotherapy is impossible 
or inadvisable, the TAT may be useful as a guide to directive therapy. 

We have also examined some of the limitations in the use of this instru- 
ment in diagnosis and therapy. It may intensify or precipitate acute attacks 
of anxiety. Moreover, when the individual is acutely disturbed, the TAT 
stories may reflect no more than his anxiety state; the content of this anxiety, 
its dynamics and origins may be completely absent from the stories. Finally, 
there are many individuals who cannot or will not assimilate interpretations 
based on their TAT protocol. The readiness of the individual to assimilate 
such interpretation must be assessed with the caution essential in any type of 
psychotherapy. 



REFERENCES 

1. Amen, E. W.: Individual differences in apperceptive reaction: A study of the 
response of preschool children to pictures. Genet. Psychol. Monograph, 23: 319-385, 
1941. 

2. Balken, E. R., and Masserman, J. H.: The language of fantasy: III. The language 
of the fantasies of patients with conversion hysteria, anxiety state, and obsessive- 
compulsive neurosis. J. Psychol., 10: 75-86, 1940. 

3. , and Van der Veer, A. H.: The clinical application of the Thematic Apperception 

Test to neurotic children. Psychol. Bull., 37: 517, 1940 (abstr.) 

4- : Protective techniques for the study of personality: a critique. Psychol. Bull., 

33: 596, 1941. 
5. , and Van der Veer, A. H.: The clinical application of a test of imagination to 

neurotic children. Am. J. Orthopsychiat., 12: 68-80, 1942. 

6. : A delineation of schizophrenic language and thought in a test of imagination. 

J. Psychol., 16: 239-271, 1943. 

7. : Thematic Apperception. J. Psychol., 20: 189-197, 1945. 

8. Bellak, L.: Analysis of the Thematic Apperception Test. Cambridge: Harvard 
Psychological Clinic (mimeographed). 

9. : An experimental investigation of projection. Psychol Bull., 39: 489, 1942. 

10. , and Jaques, E.: On the problem of dynamic conceptualization in case studies. 

Char. & Pers., 11: 20-39, 1942. 

11. : The concept of projection: an experimental investigation and study of the 

concept. Psychiatry, 7: 353~370 *944- 

12. Bennett, G.: Some factors related to substitute value of activities in normal and 
schizophrenic persons: a technique for investigation of central areas of the personality. 
Char. & Pers., 10: 42-50, 1941. 

13. : Some factors related to substitute value at the level of fantasy. Psychol. Bull., 

39: 488, 1942. 

14. Berman, P. M.: A method for diagnosing social relations from the Thematic 
Apperception Test. Radcliffe College (unpublished thesis), 1943. 

15. Bishop, F., and Kelly, G. A.: A projective method of personality investigation. 
Psychol. Bull, 39: 599, 1942. 

1 6. Brenman, M.: The recall of fairy talcs in normal and hypnotic states. Psychol. Bull., 
39: 488, 1942. 

17. Brittain, H. L.: A study in imagination. Ped. Sem., 14: 137-207, 1907. 

18. Cattell, R. B.: Projection and the design of projective tests of personality. Char. & 
Pers., 12: I77-I94 *944- 

19. Christenson, J. A.: Clinical application of the Thematic Apperception Test J. 
Abnorm. & Sodal Psychol., 38: 104-106, 1943. 

20. Clark, L. P.: The phantasy method of analyzing narcissistic neurosis. Med. J. & 
Rec., 123: 154-158, 1926. 

21. Clark, R.: A method of administering and evaluating the Thematic Apperception 
Test in group situations. Genet. Psychol. Monograph, 30: 1-55, 1944* 

22. Combs, A. W.: A method of analysis for the Thematic Apperception Test and 
autobiography. J. Clin, Psychol., 2: 167-174, 194& 

287 



^88 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

23. : The validity and reliability of interpretation from autobiography and Thematic 

Apperception Test J. Clin. Psychol., 2: 240-247, 1946. 

24. ; The use of personal experience in Thematic Apperception Test story plots. 

J. Clin. Psychol., 2: 357-363, 1946. 

25. : A comparative study of motivations as revealed in Thematic Apperception 

stories and autobiography. J. Clin. Psychol., 3: 65-74, 1947. 

26. Deabler, H. L.: Use of Murray's "need psychology" in college personnel work. 
Psychol. Bull., 39: 431, 1942. 

27. Ebbinghaus, H.: Ueber cine neue Methode zur Prufung geistiger Fahigkeiten und 
ihre Anwendung bei Schulkindern. ZPs, 13: 401-459, 1897. 

28. Finkelstein, H.: Contributions of the thematic apperception test to the understanding 
of parent-son relationships. RadclifFe College (unpublished thesis), 1943. 

29. Frank, L. K.: Protective methods for the study of personality. J. Psychol., 8: 

389-4i3> 1939- 

30. Frenkel-Brunswik, E., and Sanford, R. N.: Some personality factors in anti-Semitism. 
J. Psychol., 20: 271-291, 1945. 

31. : Personality and prejudice in women. Am. Psychol., 1: 239, 1946 (abstr.) 

32. Freud, S.: General Introduction to Psychoanalysis. New York: Boni & Liveright, 
1920. 

33. Galton, F.: Brain, 2: 149-162, 1880. 

34. Garfield, S.: Clinical values of projcctivc techniques in an army hospital. J. Clin. 
Psychol., 2: 88-91, 1946. 

35. Gcrber, J. M.: Level of interpretation of children on the Thematic Apperception 
Test. Ohio State University (unpublished MJ\. thesis), 1946. 

36. Goodman, H.: The psychogenesis of conversion hysteria and anxiety states. Harvard 
University (unpublished thesis), 1944. 

37. Harrison, R.: Studies in the use and validity of the Thematic Apperception Test with 
mentally disordered patients. II. A quantitative validity study. Ill, Validation by 
the method of "blind analysis." Char. & Pers., 9: 122-133, 134-138, 1940. 

38. : The Thematic Apperception and Rorschach methods of personality investigation 

in clinical practice. J. Psychol., 15: 49-74, 1943. 

39. , and Rotter, J. B.: A note on the reliability of the Thematic Apperception Test 

J. Abnorm. & Social Psychol., 40: 97-99, 1945. 

40. Henry, W. E.: The Thematic Apperception technique in the study of culture- 
personality relations. Genet. Psychol. Monograph, 35: 1-134, 1947. 

41. Hood, J. A.: The selection of pictures for the exploration of personality by the 
projection method. University of Southern California (unpublished thesis), 1942. 

42. Horn, D.: An experimental study of the diagnostic process in the clinical investigation 
of personality. Harvard University (unpublished PhJD. thesis), 1043. 

43. Horney, K.: Our Inner Conflicts, New York: W. W. Norton & Company, 1945. 

44. Hutt, M. L.: The use of protective methods of personality measurement in army 
medical installations. J. Clin. Psychol., 1: 134-140, 1945. 

45. Jaques, E.: The clinical use of the Thematic Apperception Test with soldiers. J. 
Abnorm. 8c Social Psychol., 40: 363-375, 1945. 

46. Jung, C. G.: Studies in Word Association. (Translated by M. D. Edcr.) New York: 
Moffatt Yard & Co., 1919 

47. Kendig, I. V.: Protective techniques as a psychological tool in diagnosis. J. Clin. 
Psychopath. P&ychother., 6: 101-110, 1944. 



REVERENCES 289 

48. Klein, G.: Scoring manual for the thematic apperception test research form. Army 
Testing Program (mimeographed). 

49. Kutash, S. B.: Performance of psychopathic defective criminals on the Thematic 
Apperception Test. J. Grim. Psychopathol., 5: 319-340, 1943. 

50. Lasaga y Travieso, J. I., and Martinez-Arango, C.: Some suggestions concerning the 
administration and interpretation of the TAT. J. Psychol., 22: 117-163, 1946. 

51. Lewin, K.: A Dynamic Theory of Personality. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1935. 

52. Libby, W.: The imagination of adolescents. Am. J. Psychol., 19: 249-252, 1908. 

53. Loeblowitz-Lennard, H., and Riessman, F,: Recall hi the Thematic Apperception 
Test: an experimental investigation into the meaning of recall of phantasy with 
reference to personality diagnosis. J, Pers., 14: 41-46, 1945. 

54. MacFarlane, J. W.: Critique of projectivc techniques. Psychol. Bull., 38: 745, 1941. 

55. Markmann, R.: Predictions of manifest personality trends by a thematic analysis of 
three pictures of the thematic apperception test. Radcliffe College (unpublished 
thesis), 1943. 

56. Masserman, J. H., and Balken, E. R.: The clinical application of phantasy studies. 
J. Psychol., 6: 81-88, 1938. 

57. - , and - : The psychoanalytic and psychiatric significance of phantasy. 

Psychoanalyt Rev., 26: 343-379, 535~549> 1939- 

58. Mayman, M., and Kutner, B.: Reliability of a procedure used in analyzing Thematic 
Apperception Test stories. (In preparation.) 

59. Meadow, A.: An analysis of Japanese character structure. 73 pp. 

60. Mill, J. S.: A System of Logic. London, 1843. 

61. Mitchell, S.: The relationship between the TAT and overt behavior, Radcliffe 
College (unpublished thesis), 1943. 

62. Morgan, C. D., and Murray, H. A.: A method for investigating phantasies: the 
Thematic Apperception Test. Arch. Neurol. & Psychiat., 34: 289-306, 1935. 

63. Murray, H. A.: Explorations in Personality. New York: Oxford University Press, 
1938, 761 pp. 

64. - 9 and Bellak, L.: Thematic Apperception Test blank. Cambridge: Harvard 
Psychological Clinic, 1941 (mimeographed). 

65. - : Thematic Apperception Test direction. Cambridge: Harvard Psychological 
Clinic, 1942 (mimeographed), 9 pp. 

66. - : Thematic Apperception Test manual. Cambridge: Harvard University Press, 
1943, 20 pp. 

6 7 . - 9 and Stein, M.: Note on the selection of combat officers. Psychosom. Mcd., 



5: 386-39i> I943- 

68. Oppenheimer, F.: Pamela: a case study in status symbols. J. Abnorm. & Social 
Psychol., 40: 187-194, 1945- 

69. Rapaport, D.: The Thematic Apperception Test: qualitative conclusions as to its 
interpretation. Psychol. Bull., 39: 592, 1942. 

70. - : Principles underlying projectivc techniques. Char. & Pers., 10: 213-219, 1942. 

71. - : The clinical application of the Thematic Apperception Test. Bull. Menainger 
Clin., 7: 106-113, 1943- 

72. - : Diagnostic Psychological Testing. Chicago: The Year Book Publishers, 1946, 
vol. 2, 516 pp. 

73. Rautman, A. L., and Brower, E.: War themes in children's stories. J. Psychol., 19: 
191202, 1945. 



290 THE THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST 

74. Renaud, H.: Group differences in fantasies: head injuries, psychoneurotics, and brain 
diseases. J. PsychoL, 21: 327-346, 1946. 

75. Richardson, L. H.: The personality of stutterers. PsychoL Monograph, 56: 41 pp. 
1944. 

76. Rodnick, E. H., and KlebanofF, S. G.: Projective reactions to induced frustrations as 
a measure of social adjustment PsychoL BulL, 39: 489, 1942. 

77. Rosenzweig, S., and Sarason, S.: An experimental study of the triadic hypothesis 
reaction to frustration, ego-defense and hypnotizability. I. Correlational approach, 
II. Thematic Apperception approach. Char. & Pers., 11: 1-19, 150-165, 1942. 

78. : Fantasy in personality and its study by test procedures. J. Abnorm. & Social 

PsychoL, 37: 1012, 1942. 

79. Rotter, J. B.: Studies in the use and validity of the Thematic Apperception Test 
with mentally disordered patients. I. Method of analysis and clinical problems. 
Char. & Pers., 9: 18-34, 1940. 

80. : Thematic Apperception Test: Suggestions for administration and interpretation 

J, Pers. 15: 70-92, 1946. 

81. Sanford, F. H.: Speech and personality: a comparative case study. Char. & Pers.. 
10: 169-198, 1942. 

82. Sanford, R. N.: Thematic Apperception Test directions for administration and 
scoring. Cambridge: Harvard Psychological Clinic, 1939 (mimeographed), 48 pp. 

83. : Some quantitative results from the analysis of children's stories. PsychoL 

BulL, 38: 749, 1941- 

84. et al.; Physique, personality and scholarship. Monographs of the Society for 

Research in Child Development, 8: 705 pp., 1943. 

$5. Sarason, S. B., and Rosenzweig, S.: An experimental study of the triadic hypothesis- 
reaction to frustration, ego-defense, and hypnotizability. II. Thematic apperception 
approach. Char. & Pers., 11: 150-165, 1942. 

86. : The use of the Thematic Apperception Test with mentally deficient children- 

I. A study of high grade girls. Am. J. Ment. Deficiency, 47: 414-421, 1943. 

87. : Dreams and Thematic Apperception Test stories. J. Abnorm. & Social PsychoL 

39: 486-492,, 1944- 

88. : The use of the Thematic Apperception Test with mentally deficient children 

H. A study of high grade boys. Am. J. Mcnt. Deficiency, 48: 169-173, 1944. 

89. : Projective techniques in mental deficiency. Char. & Pers., 13: 237-245, 1945 

90. Sargent, H.: Projective 'methods: their origins, theory, and application in personality 
research. PsychoL BulL, 5: 257-293, 1945. 

91. Schwartz, L. A.: Social-situation pictures in the psychiatric interview. Paper 
presented at the meeting of the American Orthopsychiatric Association, 1931. 

92. : Social situation pictures in the psychiatric interview. Am. J. OrthopsychiaL 

2: 124-132, 1932. 

93. Shakow, D., Rodnick, E. H., and Lebeaux T.: A psychological study of a 
schizophrenic: exemplification of a method. J. Abnorm. & Social PsychoL, 40: 
154-174, 1945. 

94. Sluts, M.: The unique contributions of the Thematic Apperception Test to a 
developmental study. PsychoL Bull., 38: 704, 1941. 

95. Symoiids, P. M.: Criteria for the selection of pictures for the investigation of 
adolescent phantasies. J. Abnorm. & Social PsychoL, 34: 271-274, 1939. 

96. : Adolescent phantasy. PsychoL BulL, 38: 596, 1941. 



REFERENCES 2pl 

97. Temple, R., and Amen, E. W.: A study of anxiety reactions in young children by 
means of a projective technique. Genet. Psychol. Monograph, 30: 59-114, 1944. 

98. Tomkins, S. S.: Limits of material obtainable in the single case study by daily 
administration of the Thematic Apperception Test. Psychol. Bull., 39: 490, 1942. 

99. : Reliability of repeated TAT's as a function of the temporal interval between 

administrations, (unpublished study.) 

100. Vernon, M. D.: The relation of cognition and phantasy in children! Brit. J. Psychol. 
Part I, 30: 273-295, 1940; Part II, 31: 1-22, 1940. 

101. Wells, H.: Differences between delinquent and non-delinquent boys as indicated 
by the Thematic Apperception Test. Psychol. Bull., 42: 534, 1945. 

1 02. White, R. W.: Prediction of hypnotic suggestibility from a knowledge of subject's 
attitudes. J. Psychol., 3: 265277, 1937. 

103. , and Sanford, R. N: Thematic Apperception Test. Cambridge: Harvard 

Psychological Clinic, 1941 (mimeographed), 15 pp. 

104. : The personality of Joseph Kidd. Char. & Pers., 11: 183-208, 318360, 1943. 

105. : Interpretation of imaginative productions. In Personality and Behavior 

Disorders, by J. McV. Hunt, pp. 235239. New York: Ronald Press, 1944. 

1 06. , Tomkins, S. S., and Alper T. G.: The realistic synthesis: a personality study. 

J. Abnorm. 8c Social Psychol., 40: 228-248, 1945. 

107. Wolf stein, M.: The reality principles in story preferences of neurotics and psychotics. 
Char. & Pcrs., 13: 135151, 1944. 

1 08. Wyatt, F.: Formal aspects of the Thematic Apperception Test. Psychol.. Bull., 
39: 491, 1942. 

I09> : Advances in the technique of the Thematic Apperception Test. Psychol. Bull., 

42: 532, 1945- 
II0 . : Personality diagnosis hi psychosomatic disturbances. Am. Psychol., 1: 264, 1946 

(abstr.) 



INDEX 



Active figures, identification with, 256, 

265 

Activity, 166 
Administration, 21-24 

effectiveness, 25 

inquiry, 24 

instructions, 2 iff 

lapport, 23 

recording, 24 

rejection, 22 

set-up, 21 

Affect and intensity, HI, 255, 265 
Age trends, 2 

in parent-child relationships, I32f 
Aggression, 8, 10, 27, 83-87, Spf, 90-102, 

148, i67E, 236, 266 
Agreement and difference, joint method 

of, 44f 

Agreement, method of, 43f 
Alper, T. G., 134 
Ambiguous figures, interpretation of, 

1 10, 256, 265 
Ambivalence, 89ff 
Antisocial behavior 

and overt behavior, 227-232 

dimensions of, 201-221 

direction, 205f, 232 

duration, 210, 232 

form, 20if, 232 

impact on other regions, 325ff, 233 

interpretation, 221-232, 233 

motivation, 202-205, 2 3 2 

role of hero, 206-210, 232 
sequelae, 211-221, 232 

temporal characteristics, 225, 233 
Antisocial wishes, 266 
Anxiety, 57, 64, 103, 104, 227, 236", 267, 

284, 286 
Applause, 237 
Atonement, 221 
Awareness, analysis of, 68-71, 105 



Balken, E., 15 
Barrier solution, 25of 



Behavior, level of, 58, 63, 103, 

Bellak, L., 8, 12 

Binet, Alfred, i 

Blank card, instruction, 23 

Brain pathology, 10, 15, 27, 79 

Brint, i48f 

Brittain, H. L., 2 



Cathesis, 279-282, 286 
Cathexis of means, 249ff 
Causes, two factors, both necessary and 
sufficient, 46ff 

either of two factors but not both, 
4 8f 

either or both of two factors, 49f 
Central value, 51, 190 
Clark, L. P., 2 
Clark, R., 5 

Concomitant variation, method of, 45f 
Condition abundance, 202 
Condition lack, 203, 204 
Conditions, definition, 31 f 

objects, 34 
Conflict, 74ff, 106, 111-114, 151, 1541", 

2i4ff, 257 

Coombs, A. W., 4, 10 
Creativity, 237 

D 

Daydreams, level of (See Phantasy) 
Dependence, 153, 236 
Depression, 169 
Diagnosis, 266-269 
Difference, method of, 44 
Differentiation 

of means and ends, 24^ 

of older figures, 123 
Dimensions 

of love and marital region, 153-170 

of sex region, 170-184 
Dimensions of analysis, 26-29 

functional interrelations, 194-198, 
200 

generality vs. specificity of concepts, 

26ff 

Direct reference, 109, 151, 176, 255, 265 



293 



294 



INDEX 



Directive therapy (See Therapy) 
Disruption of love relationship 
dimensions, 195, i96ff 

related to variation in other dimen- 
sions, 195, i96ff 

sequelae, 164-170 

source, i6if 

Distance (See Remoteness) 
Dominance, 115-118, 126, I4of, 154 
Dreams (See Validity of TAT, 101) 
Duration 

of love relationship, 158-161 

of reformation, 22of 

of sex relationship, 178 

E 

Early picture interpretation tests, 25 

Ebbinghaus, H., i 

Effectiveness (See Administration) 

Eggman, 134, 139 

Ego Ideal, 118, 254, 265 

Endogenous inhibition, 183^ 199 

Endogenous punishment, 218 

Endurance, 252^, 265 

Events, level of, 64 

Exhibitionism, 172 

Exogenous inhibition, i82f, 199 

Exogenous punishment, 211-218 
attitude toward, 213-216 
nature and duration, 2i2f 
relatedness of crime and punish- 
ment, 2I7f 
success or failure of resistance, 2i6f 

Experimentally-induced change (See 
Reliability) 

Extensity, 73f, 8 if, 106 

Extraverts, 58, 63, 103, 104 



Fame, 236, 237 
Family 

conflict with other regions, 111-114, 

*5* 

direct reference to, 109, 151 
intensity of affect, in, 151 
number and length of stories, no, 

151 
parental impact within family, 114- 

123, 151 

Feeling of inadequacy, 179, 183; (See 
also Inferiority) 



Frank, I49f 

Free association, i, 72, 102 
Freud, S., 72, 76, 101, 109 
Future security, 240 



Galton, F., i 
Gratuities, 63, 104 
Grief, 64, 104, 279 
Guilt, 64, 104, 179, 227 



Harrison, F., 4, 12, 13, 14 
Harvard Psychological Clinic, 3, 5 
Helmler, 134-139 
Help as work motive, 238 
Henry, W. F., 12, 14, 25, 103 
Hero, 124 

as instigator of antisocial behavior, 

2o6f 

Heterosexual relationships, 170-172 
History of TAT, 3f 
Homosexual relationships, 172-175 
Horn, D., 13 
Horney, K., 29 



Impact 

of family in other regions, 119-123 
of parents within family, 114-119 
of work on other regions, 258, 265 

Impotence, 179, 183 

Impulsivity, 63 

Inferiority, 85-87, 204, 235 

Inhibition, 63, 105 

of sexual behavior, 178-184 

Inquiry (See Administration) 

Intensity 73^ 8if, 106, in, 255, 265 
of love need, 157 
of sex need, i76f 

related to variation in other dimen- 
sions, 194, 195, i96f 

Introversion, 58, 103, 185 

Invariance, 52 

of behavioral level, 58, 63, 103 
of level of feeling, thought, mood 
or memory, 58, 103, 191, 
200, 222 



INDEX 



Karol, 142-147 
Kidd, Joseph, 7, 71 
Klebanoff, S. G., 8 
Kutash, S. B., 59, 103, 227 
Kutner, B., 4 



Lans, 

Lasaga y Travieso, J. I., 22 

Levels, definition of, 3o 

and work, 241-245 

cause and effect relations, ^glS 9 103 

invariance of, 57$, 103 

objects, 33f 

relative frequency of, 59, 103 

sequence analysis of, 61-68, 1046 
Lewin, K., 29 
Libby, W., 2 
Love region, 124 

definition of situation, 153-156, 
I94ff, 198 

dimensions of, 153-170 

disruption of, I55f, 161-170, I98f 

intensity, 157, 198 

reciprocity, 1561, 198 

temporal characteristics, 157-161, 
198 

M 

Maintenance of love relationship, i62ff 

Marine, case of, 57 

Markmann, R., 10, 138 

Marna, 139-142 

Masochism, 172 

Masserman, J., 15 

Masturbation, 175 

Maturation 

of love relationship, I57f 

of sex relationship, I77f 
Mayman, M., 4 
Means, cathexis of, 24911 
Means-ends, 128 



cognizance, 
differentiation of, 
elaboration, 247fE 

Memory, 64 

Mental defectives, 10 

Mill, J. S., 43 

Morgan, C. D., 3, 12 



Motivation 

in work region, 234-241 

of antisocial behavior, 202-205 
Multifactor analysis, 50$ 

invariance, 52 

necessary conditions, 52f, 184, 200 

threat, 5if 

values, 51 

Murray, H. A., 3, 10, 12, 14, 21, 22, 23, 
24, 25, 26, 29, 56, 57, 103 

N 

Nancy, 184-190 
Navaho, 12, 14, 25, 59, 103 
Necessary conditions, 52f, 191 
Need-press analysis, 26f, 55!! 
Number and length of stories, iro, 255, 

265 



Object description, level of, 103 
Older figures, no, 123, 151 
Organization, 251 
Overt and covert needs, 55ff, iO2f 



Parent-child relationships 
changes in, I32ff, i5if 
generalization of, 123-131, 151 

Parental figures, 

impact on other regions, 114-123, 

151 
interpretation of ambiguous figures, 

no, 151 
interpretation of older figures, no, 

151 

introduction of, io9f, 151 

omission of older figures; no, 151 
Passive figures, omission of, 256, 265 
Passive pictures, restructuration, 256, 265 
Passivity, 62f 

rejection of, 257, 265 
Personality stability (See Reliability) 
Phantasy, level of, 241!!, 248f, 264 
Physical handicap, 236 
Play therapy (See Therapy) 
Poverty, 202, 203, 235 
Power motive, 240 
Prediction (See Validity of TAT) 



296 



INDEX 



Pressure, 73-76, 106 

estimate of pressure of repressed 
wish, 82 

estimate of pressure of repressing 
wish, 82 

estimate of ratio of conflicting pres- 
sures to total pressure, 80 

ratio of conflicting pressures, 74f, 

ratio of conflicting pressures and 
ratio of combined pressures 
to total pressure, 75f 
relation of conflicting pressures to 

total pressure, 74, 107 
Psychoanalysis, 12, 75f 
Psychoneuroses, 10, 15, 27, 76 
Psychopathic defective criminal, 59, 103, 

227 

Psychoses, 15, 76 
Punishment, 211-219 



Q 



Qualifiers, 



Rapaport, D., 21, 22, 24, 25 

Realism, 246 

Reciprocity in love relationship, is6f 

as related to variation in other di- 
mensions, 194, 195, 196 

in sexual relationship, i75f 
Reconciliation, 163, 164 
Recording (See Administration) 
Reformation, 163, 2i9f 
Rejection, 183, 204, 235 
Reliability, 4-9 

and elapsed interval, 6 

and experimentally induced change, 
8f 

and personality stability, ji 

interpreter reliability, 4f 

repeat reliability, 6-9 
Remorse, 16$, 180 
Remoteness, 27, 78^, 107 

estimate of degree, 81 
Renaud, H, n, 15, 26, 79 
Repayment, 239 



Repression, 72-84, i05f 

elicting repressed memories, 270$ 

history, 72 

identification of repressed wishes, 
82 

measurement, 77-82, ror 

theory, 73-76, 106 
Resistance, 72, 2i3f 
Responsibility, acceptance of, 
Richardson, L. H., 15 
Rivalry, 238 
Rodnick, E. H., 8 
Rorschach test, 12 
Rotter, J. B., 4, 13, 21 



Sadism, 172 

Sanford, R. N., 4, 7, 26, 55, 56, 57, i02f 

Sarason, S. B., n 

Schwartz, L. A., 3 

Scoring sample, 34-41 

Sequence analysis (See Levels) 

Sex, 90-102, 266 

Sex differences, 2 

Sex region 

dimensions, 170-184 

hetrosexual relationships, 170-172 

homosexual relationships, 172-175, 
199 

inhibition, 178-184, 199 

reciprocity, i75f, 199 

temporal characteristics, I77f, 199 

type of need, 170-175 
Shame, 64, 104 
Slutz, M., 4 
Social context of antisocial behavior, 



Social region, 124 

Social Situation Picture Test, 3 

Socio-economic status, 240 

Special state, 31, 203 

Spiritual fulfillment, 239 

Stein, M. I., 14 

Strivers, introduction of, 2551, 265 

Stutterers, 15 

Substitute satisfactions, i66 

Suicide, i69f, 185 



INDEX 



297 



Super-ego, 77, 222 
Symonds, P. M., n 

T 

Temporal characteristics, 157-161, 1771!, 

251-254 

related to variation in other dimen- 
sions, 195, 196, 197 

Therapy, 270-286 

attitudes toward, 272-275, 286 
directive therapy, 282-284, 286 
instrument for catharsis, 279-282, 

286 

limitations, 284$ 
play therapy, 275-279, 286 

Thought, level of, 244f 

Threat, 51, I9of 

Time perspective, 25if, 265 

Tomkins, S. S., 134 



Unification, principle of, 28f 



Validity of TAT, 9-19 
and case study, 13 
and dreams, nf 
and groups of known difference, 

13-16 

and past history, lof 
and prediction, 16 
and psychoanalysis, 12 
and Rorschach test, 12 
increased validity in conjunction 

with other procedures, 16- 

19 

Values, 51 

Variability 

interpretation of, 192-194 
range of, 192, 200 



Vector 

"against", 169, 172, 201, 203, 204 
"away from", 202 

' V, 154, I7i 

"for", 154, 171, 202 

"from", 155, 172, 201, 202, 203, 204 

"on", 153, 170 

"over", 154, 171, 202 

"toward", 155, 171, 172 

"under", 203, 204 

"with", 155, 171 
Vectors, definition of, 29f 

objects, 33 
Voyeurism, 171 

W 

White, R. W., 7, 18, 71, 134 

Wish 

instigators of, 64-68, I04f 
sequelae of, 6iff, 104 

Work habits, 254 

Work region, 124, 165 

cathexis of means, 249-251, 264 
complexity of organization, 251, 264 
conditions of, 258-263, 265 
differentiation of means-ends, 245f 
importance of, 254-258, 265 
level analysis, 241-245, 264 
means-end cognizance, 246-249, 264 
motivation of, 234-241, 264 
temporal characteristics, 251-254, 



"X", 83-87, 108 



'T', 87-90, 108 



"Z", 7f, n, 13, 90-102, 108, 224, 270$ 
273