Skip to main content

Full text of "Thinking with blacks: a diiscussion of the use of black human forms by the white mind"

See other formats


Yale  University  Library 


MUDD 

LIBRARY 


Medical 


YALE 


MEDICAL  LIBRARY 


Permission  for  photocopying  or  microfilming  of  11 


(TITLE  OF  THESIS) 


for  the  purpose  of  individual  scholarly  consultation  or  reference  is  hereby 
granted  by  the  author.  This  permission  is  not  to  be  interpreted  as  affect¬ 
ing  publication  of  this  work  or  otherwise  placing  it  in  the  public  domain, 
and  the  author  reserves  all  rights  of  ownership  guaranteed  under  common 
law  protection  of  unpublished  manuscripts. 


Digitized  by  the  Internet  Archive 
in  2017  with  funding  from 
Arcadia  Fund 


https://archive.org/details/thinkingwithblacOObege 


Thinking  with  Blacks: 

A  Discussion  of  the  Use  of  Black  Human  Forms  by  the  White  Mind 


Daniel  M«  Begel,M4 


Contents 


Chapter  I  page  1 

Chapter  II  16 

Chapter  III  46 

Appendix  62 

Bibliography  71 


i 


I 


The  Study  of  Race  Symbolization 

The  psychological  study  of  race  symbolization  has  focused  on 
"prejudice"  and  ,!racism0"  "Prejudice"  is  defined  by  Gordon  Allport 
as  "an  avertive  or  hostile  attitude  toward  a  person  who  belongs  to 
a  group,  simply  because  he  belongs  to  that  group,  and  is  therefore 
presumed  to  have  the  objectionable  qualities  ascribed  to  the  group" 
(Allport,  1954).  "Racism,"  a  concept  often  used  interchangeably 
with  prejudice  and  rarely  defined  by  writers  who  study  it,  is 
usually  thought  of  as  prejudice  in  action.  Kovel  says  "race  prej¬ 
udice  ...is  clearly  a  causal  agent  in  racism"  (Kovel,  1970). 

Although  racist  action  presumably  depends  upon  a  hostile 
attitude  associated  with  a  belief  in  negative  qualities,  we  should 
remember  that  it  is  possible  for  a  person  to  have  an  ambivalent  or 
even  friendly  attitude  toward  an  objectionable  object,  or  contra¬ 
dictory  beliefs  about  an  object  of  hostility.  Robert  Coles,  for 
example,  sensitively  reports  in  his  long  and  beautiful  quotations 
from  conversations  with  white  Southerners  the  complex  feelings  of 
tenderness  and  anger,  superiority  and  self -recrimination  (Coles, 
1964) . 

But  on  the  whole  the  psychological  literature  on  race  divides 
the  operations  of  the  mind  according  to  moral  criteria.  Where 
the  question  concerns  whites  thinking  about  blacks,  five  aspects 
have  been  identified.  First,  the  black  is  evaluated  negatively. 
Second,  the  white  is  evaluated  positively.  Third,  there  is  a 
tendency  toward  avoidance  and/or  fourth,  there  is  a  tendency  to- 


2 


ward  domination.  Fifth,  there  is  a  tendency  toward  hate.  These 
are  the  criteria  which  alone  or  in  some  combination  define  an  idea 
as  11  racist."  This  is  the  sense  in  which  we  will  use  the  term  in 
this  paper. 

Writers  who  are  struck  by  the  power  and  apparent  ubiquity  of 
such  ideas  in  American  society  have  tended  to  assert  that  the  black 
embodies  particular  universal  meanings  for  whites,  Lawrence  Kubie 
states  that  race  prejudice  has  its  roots  in  three  "nearly  universal" 
experiences  of  childhood  (Kubie,  19 65),  The  origin  of  anti-black 
prejudice  he  highlights  is  the  child’s  oscillation  between  secret 
guilty  pride  in  his  body  and  a  hidden  aversion  to  it.  A  complex 
web  of  feelings — guilt,  fascination,  fear,  and  loathing— is  asso¬ 
ciated  particularly  with  feces,  "We  teach  the  child  that  his  body 
is... a  dirt  factory.,, A  child’s  buried  feelings  of  disgust  with 
himself  is  one  of  his  later  reactions  against  differences."  The 
thought  that  "the  other  is  dirty.. .(is)  a  defense  against  a  con¬ 
ception  of  a  dirty  self,"  The  other  race  represents  feces  and  re¬ 
lated  symbols, 

J.W.  Hamilton  follows  Kubie  in  an  interesting  discussion  of  a 
Midwestern  university  town  which  reacted  to  an  open  housing  drive 
with  a  cleanup  drive  and  a  crusade  against  homosexuals  (Hamilton, 
1966)0  He  sees  anti-black  prejudice  as  a  defense  against  the  threat 
of  the  Negro,  who  represents  feces,  to  anal  compulsivi ty »  He  is 
slightly  more  rigid  than  Kubie  about  the  black’s  meaning  to  whites 
and  expands  the  list  of  associations.  According  to  Hamilton,  white 
represents  up,  breast,  good,  milk,  God,  heaven,  and  clean,  while 
black  represents  all  the  opposites . 


,  .  . 


3 


James  Comer  and  Richard  Sterba  add  to  this  list  of  meanings . 
They  point  out  that  blacks  can  represent  the  principals  of  the 
Oedipal  situation,  Sterba  sees  general  anti-Negro  feeling  as  being 
based  on  the  representation  of  younger  siblings  by  the  black  man 
(Sterba,  19^7).  He  associates  attacking  behavior  with  "collective 
father  murder"  and  the  emergence  of  repressed  father  hatred.  Comer 
adds  that  the  black  man  can  also  represent  the  powerless  self  of 
the  Oedipal  situation,  or  the  repressed  aggressive  self  of  that 
time  (Comer,  1970),  West  seconds  this  idea:  "Put  into  (the  Negro) 
these  (sexual)  parts  of  yourself  and  you  become  a  part  of  him  as  you 
now  imagine  him  to  be.,, Thus  arises  the  secret  wish  that  Negro  men 
will  actually  transgress  successfully  against  white  women"  (West, 
1967).  Pinderhughes  states  "The  value  orientation  and  psychology 
associated  with  a  class  or  caste  divisions  are  derived  from  pro¬ 
jections  of  the  body  image"  (Pinderhughes,  1969),  "High-type" 
groups  are  associated  with  the  head,  the  brain,  and  incorporation 
while  "low-type"  groups  are  associated  with  the  genitals  and  anus, 
with  expulsion,  with  what  should  be  kept  out  by  the  bottom,  includ¬ 
ing  not  only  forbidden  objects  but  forbidden  thoughts. 

There  are  important  limitations  to  this  approach  to  race 
symbolization.  To  begin  with,  a  symbolic  form  is  intrinsically 
ambiguous  in  meaning,  as  the  wide  range  cited  by  the  above  authors 
suggests.  The  significance  of  an  object  of  prejudice  can  vary  from 
person  to  person.  Ackerman  and  Jahoda, in  a  study  of  twenty-seven 
anti-Semitic  patients,  discuss  the  inconsistent  stereotype  of  the 
Jew  (Ackerman  and  Jahoda,  19^8).  They  state  "the  specific  selec¬ 
tion  that  an  individual  makes  out  of  this  wealth  of  contrasting 
attributes  can  be  understood  only  if  this  selection  is  discussed 


, 


4 


simultaneously  with  the  individual  anti-Semite’s  attitude  toward 
his  own  self."  And  as  Coles’  study  shows,  the  significance  can 
change  by  the  moment  for  a  single  individual.  That  is,  a  prejudiced 
person,  like  anyone  else,  may  be  ambivalent. 

True,  the  meanings  of  race  derive  in  part  from  universal  child¬ 
hood  experiences.  But  this  does  not  distinguish  race  symbolization 
from  any  other  symbolization  of  people.  Childhood  may  be  a  neces¬ 
sary  condition  for  race  symbolization,  but  as  an  explanation  of 
racism  it  is  insufficient.  For  racism  can  emerge,  disappear,  and 
change  content  long  after  childhood  is  over,  Brian  Bird  reported 
a  two  week  episode  of  anti-Negro  prejudice  in  a  liberal,  anxious, 
phobic,  Jewish  female  with  a  highly  developed  sense  of  social  con¬ 
science  (Bird,  1957) •  During  therapy  she  developed  the  belief 
that  Negro  men  were  inferior,  were  envious  of  whites,  and  would 
get  out  of  control  and  try  to  elevate  themselves  by  having  sex  with 
white  women.  These  beliefs  were  coupled  with  hatred  and  fear. 

The  patient  was  developing  a  positive  transference  at  that  time 
and  wished  to  "cross  the  gulf"  between  herself  and  her  analyst. 

Her  prejudice  was  a  "final  defense"  against  the  realization  of 
these  feelings  in  the  face  of  the  analyst’s  imagined  resentment  of 
her.  It  disappeared  after  these  feelings  were  analyzed. 

In  another  case  study,  Terry  Rogers  discusses  a  passive,  ob¬ 
sessive  white  Southerner  who  became  an  active  anti-Negro  racist  and 
subsequently  dropped  psychoanalysis  to  become  a  leader  of  a  White 
Citizen’s  Council  (Rogers,  i960).  Rogers  sees  this  development  as 
a  protection  against  emerging  homosexual  wishes,  incestuous  desires, 
and  patricidal  impulses,  involving  the  "wholesale  use  of  the  de¬ 
fense  mechanisms  of  projection  and  identif ication  with  the  aggres- 


. 

5 


sor."  Like  Sterba,  he  says  "in  the  unconscious  of  white  people, 
the  Negro  male  represents  the  hated  father,  particularly  the 
father  at  night," 

The  defense  of  projection  is  emphasized  by  virtually  every 
student  of  race  symbolization.  Pinderhughes ,  for  example,  speaks 
of  "projections  of  the  body  image."  And  Brian  Bird  speaks  of 
" incorpro jection, "  He  says  the  prejudiced  individual  or  group 
"borrows  imagined  and  feared  indignation  from  a  "higher8  race,  and 
loans  its  own  guilt  (really  repressed  wishes)  to  the  "lower"  race." 
Thus,  a  conflict  is  passed  "right  through"  the  ego.  Bird"s  fine 
piece  stresses  the  adaptive,  ego-syntonic  nature  of  prejudice.  He 
sees  it  as  giving  vent  to  hostility  as  well  as  keeping  aggressive 
thought  from  being  acted  out.  Certain  limitations  to  the  notion 
of  " incorpro jection"  are  suggested,  however,  by  the  generalizations 
Bird  derives  from  this  notion.  For  example,  the  statement  that  prej¬ 
udice  will  not  occur  in  "successful"  people  and  groups  is  contra¬ 
dicted  by  the  racist  beliefs  of  Thomas  Jefferson  (Jordan,  1968)  and 
the  anti -Communism  of  numerous  American  Administrations  (New  York 
Times .  almost  any  year). 

The  concept  of  projection  has  even  more  severe  limitations, 
however,  as  an  explanation  of  race  prejudice  or  race  symbolization. 
When  used  in  the  narrow  sense  of  the  attribution  of  one"s  own  id 
impulses  to  another  it  fails  to  explain  a  whole  host  of  qualities 
attributed  to  blacks,  such  as  that  blacks  are  musical,  or  natural, 
or  lazy.  When  used  in  the  broad  sense  of  externalization,  it  only 
implies  that  external  perceptions  depend  upon  something  known  in¬ 
ternally.  In  other  words,  the  psychological  source  of  racism  is 
psychological . 


11  .  '  o  ? 


. 

6 


Rubenf eld,  in  an  unpublished  doctoral  dissertation  done  at 
Yale  in  1963,  attempted  to  prove  six  hypotheses  based  on  the  pro¬ 
jection-prejudice  hypothesis,  with  respect  to  sexual  conflict.  He 
used  both  the  standard  TAT  pictures  and  a  set  of  redrawings  very 
similar  to  the  standard  set  except  the  figures  were  black.  The 
blacks  in  the  redrawings  were  dressed  the  same  as  whites,  had  wavy 
hair,  and  were  not  very  dark.  Some  of  the  results  were  interesting. 
For  example,  one  standard  picture  elicited  stories  of  sexual  con¬ 
flict  from  a  majority  of  white  subjects,  but  when  redrawn  elicited 
stories  in  which  the  male  figure  was  seen  as  leaving  to  perform 
some  aggressive  act,  Rubenf eld  concluded  that  this  was  an  artifact 
of  the  test  because  blacks  are  seen  culturally  as  inherently  re¬ 
bellious.  It  could  be  argued,  however,  that  blacks  have  many  cul¬ 
tural  meanings,  one  of  which  is  that  they  argue  a  lot  about  sex. 

This  meaning  could  have  been  selected  but  was  not  for  reasons 
which  are  obscure  but  worth  investigating.  Regardless  of  the  in¬ 
terpretation  of  this  change,  it  is  clear  that  blackness  had  an 
impact  on  the  stories.  However,  none  of  the  hypotheses  based  on 
the  projection-prejudice  notion  could  be  verified.  One  hypothesis 
that  failed,  for  instance,  was  that  prejudiced  people  would  project 
more  sexual  ideas  onto  Negro  figures  than  white  figures.  Another 
was  that  in  sexual  arousal  situations  high  prejudiced  people  would 
show  a  greater  increase  in  sexual  imagery  in  response  to  Negro 
cards  than  low-prejudiced  people. 

Experimentally,  the  projection-prejudice  hypothesis  doesn’t 
bear  fruit.  Logically  we  should  expect  this,  for  to  imply  that 
the  white’s  black  man  is  needed  only  as  a  "bad  man,"  however  de¬ 
fined,  suggests  a  peculiarly  simple  motivation  operating  in  race 


7 


symbolization.  There  are  other  problems  with  the  idea  of  projec¬ 
tion  onto  blacks,  to  which  we  will  return  shortly. 

First,  however,  we  should  discuss  a  work  by  Joel  Kovel: 

White  Racism,  A  Psychohistory  (Kovel,  1970),  This  book  deserves 
special  mention  for  several  reasons.  First,  it  is  the  only  book- 
length  discussion  in  the  psychological  literature  which  deals  ex¬ 
clusively  with  white  symbolization  of  blacks.  Second,  it  is  widely 
read  and  regarded  as  an  important  work.  Third,  it  demonstrates 
the  inadequacies  of  the  notions  we  have  been  discussing  and  extends 
them  to  a  degree  wisely  avoided  by  other  authors, 

Kovel’s  idea  is  that  race  fantasies  are  generated  in  the  uni¬ 
versal  setting  of  childhood  and  applied  at  second  hand  to  races  in 
a  culture  which  uses  them  to  handle  its  historical  problems.  The 
culture  is  both  an  outlet  for  and  in  part  a  projection  of  these 
fantasies.  There  is  a  "congruency  (of  the  institution  of  racism) 
with  the  personalities  of  the  people  within  society .Instinctual 
conflict,  when  projected  onto  culture,  is  one  of  the  crucial  deter¬ 
minants  of  historical  power--in  our  case,  of  white  men  over  black— 
(which)  is  the  single  most  salient  thread  of  history." 

The  psychological  origins  of  racism  develop  during  the  anal 
phase  when  fantasies  about  dirt  and  property  develop  and  first 
separation  from  the  mother  occurs.  Excrement  becomes  associated 
with  ambivalence  around  separation.  Dirt  becomes  the  recipient  of 
the  child’s  anger  at  separation;  while  the  love  of  possessions  be¬ 
comes  a  substitute  for  the  love  of  what  has  been  separated  from  him, 
"Since  racism  involves  the  separateness  of  people,  so  must  it  be 
invested  with  anal  fantasies"  (emphasis  mine,  D.B.),  In  the  phallic 
phase,  the  central  themes  are  castration  and  fantasies  about 


- 

8 


genital  activity  with  forbidden  people  in  the  setting  of  compe¬ 
tition  and  envy,  "The  resolution  of  the  Oedipus  complex  condenses 
all  the  previous  stages  of  development .under  one  mental  organ¬ 
ization,  the  superego », .By  adjusting  his  superego  to  a  set  of  cul¬ 
tural  controls,  a  person  adapts  and  becomes  "normal.®  If  he  is  a 
white  American,  it  is  likely  that  he  will  then  find  an  outlet  for 
some  of  his  infantile  fantasies  about  dirt,  property,  power,  and 
sexuality  in  his  culture’s  racism," 

The  'hversive  racist"  is  one  for  whom  anal  fantasies  are  of 
primary  importance,  A  split  of  infantile  origins  between  good  and 
bad  body  (the  self  and  what  is  expelled)  becomes  generalized. 

Objects  are  sought  to  represent  both,  "For  symbols  of  what  is 
hated,. .the  developing  mind  looks  to  see  what  can  be  associated  with 
excrement,"  while  property  becomes  the  loved  excrement. 

The  "dominative  racist"  has  a  harsher,  more  rigid  superego 
than  the  aversive  racist.  Oedipal  fantasies  are  more  significant 
to  him.  Black  symbolizes  bad  and  white  good,  "The  black  man  is 
the  bad  father  who  possesses  the  black  mammy  (who  is  herself  impure) 
and  has  the  genital  power  which  forever  excites  the  child’s  envy. 

He  is  also  the  bad  child  who  lusts  after  the  pure  and  utterly  for¬ 
bidden  white  mother.,, By  making  the  rape  fantasy  the  cornerstone 
of  his  culture,  the  white  male  only  repeats  in  adulthood  the  cen¬ 
tral  incest  taboo  of  his  childhood. . ,The  Southern  white  male 
simultaneously  resolves  both  sides  of  the  conflict  by  keeping  the 
black  man  submissive  and  by  castrating  him  when  submission  fails.., 
he  is  castrating  the  father.,, and  also  identifying  with  him  by 
castrating  the  son. , .Black  man,  white  man,  black  woman,  white  woman- 
each  realizes  some  aspect  of  the  oedipal  situation;  and  in  this 


9 


realization,  the  infantile  impossibilities  of  the  oedipal  conflicts 
attain  their  perverted  resolution  by  being  projected  onto  split 
elements  of  culture."  Further,  "in  accordance  with  the  principle 
of  multiple  function,  and  for  survival  of  the  ego  (in  the  face  of 
the  persistence  of  the  original  wish) ,  some  sense  of  realization 
is  necessary. . .The  solution  devised  by  the  ego..."  is  that  it 
"will  find  a  symbolic  representation, , .that  corresponds  with  some 
perception  of  the  world," 

We  have  already  discussed  the  assertion  that  a  symbolic  form 
"must"  be  invested  with  particular  meanings  and  found  it  wanting. 

But  in  addition  Kovel  makes  us  wonder  why  projection  should  operate 
so  ubiquitously  in  the  case  of  race.  His  answer,  that  projection 
onto  another  race  is  necessary  "for  the  survival  of  the  ego," 
ignores  the  broad  range  of  other  defenses  available  to  the  mind. 

The  "why"  of  projection  is  usually  answered  this  way,  in  terms  of 
some  fundamental  necessity.  For  example,  Pinderhughes  speaks  of 
a  "drive  to  dichotomize"  (Pinderhughes,  1971)®  And  Zilboorg, 
another  student  of  racism,  talks  of  a  "herd  instinct"  (Zilboorg, 
19^7).  He  says  that  herds  do  not  satisfy  a  need  for  togetherness, and 
are  by  nature  aggressive.  This  aggression  leads  to  guilt  and 
anxiety  which  is  dealt  with  by  projection  which  increases  fear  of 
being  alone  which  stimulates  an  attempt  at  passivity  which  arouses 
homosexual  anxiety  which  leads  to  greater  projection  which  starts 
the  vicious  cycle  all  over  again.  "The  circle  of  this  psychological 
economy  is  thus  closed,  perfect,  and  immutable .To  defend  his 
illusory  security,  man  projects  his  herdness . . .Here  lies  the  very 
secret  of  the  tenacity  and  intensity  of . „ , pre judice . "  If  this 
psychosocial  "cycle"  seems  unconvincing  in  paraphrase,  a  reading 


■ 


10 


of  the  original  is  unlikely  to  clarify.  "The  narcissistic,  anal- 
sadistic  strivings  to  gain  a  sort  of  purely  passive,  intrauterine 
existence  in  the  womb  of  the  mass,  or  nature,  are  brazenly  project¬ 
ed  into  the  enemy  and  then  aspired  to  and  approximated  by  the  pro¬ 
jector  who  is  himself  the  accuser." 

By  relying  on  such  notions  as  "outlet"  and  "survival  of  the 
ego"  we  are  able  to  avoid  many  questions.  If  repressed  universal 
instinctual  conflicts  are  what  is  projected,  how  do  qualities  that 
are  not  part  of  the  conflicts  as  Kovel  describes  them,  such  as 
laziness  or  cunning,  come  to  be  projected?  If  what  is  projected  is 
what  is  unacceptable  and  repressed,  why  is  it  that  blacks  represent 
"hated  excrement"  rather  than  the  surely  more  unacceptable  idea 
"loved  excrement?"  And  why  is  it  that  castration  anxiety  drops 
entirely  out  of  the  picture  Kovel  draws  (with  the  wish  to  castrate 
the  father  remaining)  until  the  black  man  is  actually  castrated, 
an  event  far  more  infrequent  than  the  prevalence  of  racism?  Here, 
it  suddenly  reemerges  by  being  "realized"  through  an  equation  of 
the  black  man  with  the  evil  child. 

Another  problem  for  this  theory  is  posed  by  the  existence  of 
positive  ideas  about  blacks,  such  as  the  idea  that  "blacks  are 
kinder"  (from  an  interview  with  a  friend) .  To  preserve  his  theory, 
Kovel  would  have  to  respond  to  this  statement  in  one  of  several 
ways.  First,  it  is  a  realistic  perception,  but  therefore  not  a 
projection.  Second,  It  is  a  reaction-formation  against  another 
idea.  Again,  it  is  not  a  projection  since  there  is  no  conscious 
perception  of  the  repressed  idea  in  the  external  world.  Third,  it 
is  a  projection  of  a  quality  which  is  unconsciously  unacceptable 
to  the  self,  but  acceptable  in  blacks.  The  reversal  in  this  case 


11 


requires  an  explanation  which  is  not  suggested. 

The  fourth  possibility  is  that  this  is  a  phenomenon  which 
Kovel  recognizes  but  does  not  attempt  to  explain.  He  seems  to 
think  there  are  four  kinds  of  orientation  towards  blacks:  the 
dominative  racist,  the  aversive  racist,  the  "metaracist"  and  "those 
who  treat  another  person  as  he  is.., without  the  shackles  of  cate¬ 
gories,"  If  on  the  other  hand  Kovel  admits  of  this  fifth  possibil¬ 
ity,  a  positive  orientation,  then  it  can  be  excluded  from  discussion 
only  by  treating  racism  as  a  cultural  phenomenon  consisting  of  a 
miscellaneous  amalgamation  of  attitudes  selected  on  the  basis  of  a 
value  judgment.  This  precludes  any  psychological  explanation  which 
goes  beyond  the  idea  that  if  it!s  bad,  it's  got  to  be  a  projection. 

We  have  seen  that  the  unadulterated  racism  Kovel  describes  is 
only  one  possibility  among  many  types  of  white  orientation  toward 
blacks.  According  to  Coles,  a  self-respecting  dominative  racist 
may  also  love  the  man  he  calls  black.  Compare  the  quotations  In 
Coles®  book  with  Kovel's  "Pattern:  blackness  is  bad,  what  goes  on 
in  the  dark  comes  from  the  dark;  therefore,  make  the  black  man  re¬ 
present  both  father  and  son  in  their  destructive  aspects.,,"  and 
so  forth.  Further,  bits  and  pieces  of  Kovel's  phenomenon  can  appear 
without  the  other  elements,  while  racist  beliefs  can  change,  appear 
and  disappear.  In  short,  Kovel  has  explained  a  phenomenon  which 
in  a  psychological  sense  does  not  exist  by  a  "mechanism"  which, 
when  operative  at  all,  itself  requires  an  explanation. 

The  extension  of  such  a  theory  leads  to  some  dubious  asser¬ 
tions,  For  instance,  Kovel  believes  the  white  Southern  female  was 
made  "sexless,  in  reality,"  This  is  a  curious  statement  in  light 
of  the  many  generations  of  all  white  Southerners  who  were  born--in 


. 


12 


reality.  Kovel  considers  this  hypothesis  "too  well  known  to  need 
documentation. "  And  in  fact,  the  only  documentation  for  his  theory 
at  all  is  provided  by  secondary  sources,  with  an  occasional  bit  of 
distortion.  For  example,  Kovel  labels  "virtually  psychotic"  a  man 
he  has  never  seen  and  whom  Rogers,  the  man’s  therapist,  describes 
as  "passive"  and  "obsessive." 

Similarly,  he  uses  an  excellent  historical  work  by  Winthrop  Jordan 
(1968)  primarily  as  a  source  of  historical  "facts,"  and  gives  the 
work  a  one-sided  reading.  There  is  no  sense  in  Kovel’s  work  that, 
as  Jordan  states,  "provincial  Americans ...  showed  themselves  pulled 
by  opposing  tendencies--the  need  to  explain  why  Negroes  looked  both 
the  same  as  and  different  from  white  men  and  the  twin  senses  that 
man  both  is  and  is  not  an  animal."  Jordan  talks  about  the  "puzzle" 
of  the  Negro’s  color,  and  the  "human  relationships,  continually 
driving  home  the  common  humanity  of  all"  and  "the  push  and  pull  of 
an  irreconcilable  conflict  between  desire  and  aversion  for  inter¬ 
racial  sexual  union."  The  point  is  not  that  Kovel  misconstrues 
Jordan’s  conclusions  about  the  dominant  outcome  of  these  tensions, 
but  only  that  he  is  blind  to  Jordan’s  sense  of  development  and 
conflict.  Jordan,  in  studying  white  attitudes,  allows  himself  to 
discern  both  sides  of  tensions  at  many  levels  of  consciousness. 

He  shows  how,  with  the  support  of  religious  ideas,  "scientific" 
notions,  chance,  cultural  symbolizat ions ,  and  slavery  itself,  the 
history  of  white  attitudes  grew  and  changed  under  the  impact  of  the 
needs  of  Americans  to  know  who  they  were,  and  to  solve  problems  of 
mastery,  control,  and  freedom.  That  the  solution  rested  on  a  per¬ 
ception  of  difference  and  involved  a  measure  of  projection  onto 
enslavable  and  enslaved  men  is  clear.  Had  it  involved  only  this, 


•  ■  V. 

:  - 
.  >  r  ~ 

■  ■ 


13 


Jordan’s  contribution  to  the  psychological  and  historical  under¬ 
standing  of  the  problem  would  have  been  trivial. 

Many  authors  have  asked  themselves  why  ideas  about  race  seem 
so  powerful  and  so  ubiquitous.  The  most  important  contribution  to 
the  psychological  understanding  of  this  problem  has  been  the  demon¬ 
stration  that  the  content  of  racism  may  be  motivated  by  unconscious 
ideas  derived  from  the  experiences  of  childhood  and  may  serve  a 
defensive  function.  But  this  is  true  as  well  of  symptoms,  sympto¬ 
matic  acts,  jokes,  and  fantasy  life  in  general.  The  problem  of 
race  symbolization  per  se  has  been  largely  ignored.  Recourse  to 
the  concept  of  "projection"  merely  raises  the  central  question  in 
a  different  way:  Why  projection  onto  an  entire  race  of  people? 

This  question  has  been  almost  defined  out  of  existence  by  a 
moralistic  decision.  Theories  which  assume  the  universality  of 
racism  but  ignore  the  positive  stereotypes  are  free  to  ignore  the 
process  by  which  universal  childhood  experiences  have  different 
manifestations  in  adult  race  symbolization.  Theories  which  treat 
racism  as  episodic  focus  on  the  manner  in  which  the  solution  of 
particular  psychological  problems  employ  particular  defense  mech¬ 
anisms,  But  since  these  theories  also  ignore  positive  ideas  about 
another  race,  race  symbolization  can  be  treated  as  if  it  were  only 
a  matter  of  defense, 

Robert  Coles  has  hinted  that  what  is  important  about  the  black 
person  to  whites  is  not  that  he  has  a  particular  universal  meaning, 
but  that  he  can  universally  mean  something— in  fact,  anything.  If 
this  is  true  and  if  race  symbolization  is  as  widespread  and  power¬ 
ful  as  so  many  of  us  believe,  it  may  be  that  the  various  black 
people  of  the  mind  share,  not  a  common  meaning,  but  a  common 


14 


symbolic  process  of  creation. 

To  test  this  idea  we  must  have  some  hypothesis  about  what  this 
process  might  be.  The  purpose  of  this  paper  is  to  formulate  such 
a  hypothesis  on  the  basis  of  experimental  data. 

The  data  we  will  use  was  gathered  in  a  simple  way.  One  white 
subject,  an  undergraduate  male,  was  shown  a  set  of  fourteen  Thematic 
Apperception  Test  cards.  These  were  photographs  taken  specifically 
for  this  study.  The  fourteen  consist  of  two  sets  of  seven,  one 
with  all  white  figures  (the  W  set)  and  one  with  one  black  on  each 
card  (the  B  set).  In  each  set  of  seven,  four  of  the  cards  show 
two  figures  and  three  show  a  single  figure.  For  each  card  in  the 
W  set  there  is  a  card  in  the  B  set  which  corresponds  to  it  in  terms 
of  age,  sex,  and  relative  position  of  the  figures.  The  difference 
is  in  the  substitution  of  a  black  figure  for  a  white  on  each  card 
in  the  B  set.  Thus,  four  of  the  cards  in  the  B  set  show  a  white 
figure  and  a  black  figure,  and  three  show  a  single  black  figure. 

(For  a  description  of  the  cards  together  with  the  stories  told  to 
them  see  the  Appendix.) 

The  subject  was  given  a  modification  of  the  standard  TAT  in¬ 
structions.  He  was  told: 

This  test  is  part  of  a  medical  school  thesis  I  am  doing. 
Although  I  cannot  explain  the  nature  of  the  thesis  to  you 
beforehand,  you  will  no  doubt  get  an  idea  of  the  general 
area  of  the  study  as  we  go  along.  Please  don't  concern 
yourself  too  much  with  this  while  taking  the  test.  After 
the  test  is  over,  I  will  be  interested  in  discussing  it  with 
you. 

Today  1 5 11  be  showing  you  a  series  of  pictures  and 
would  like  you  to  tell  an  imaginative,  dramatic  story  about 
each  one.  Tell  what  has  led  up  to  the  event  shown  in  the 
picture,  describe  what  is  happening  at  the  moment,  what  the 
characters  are  feeling  and  thinking,  and  then  give  the  out¬ 
come.  Speak  your  thoughts  as  they  come  to  your  mind. 

Although  it  is  unlikely,  you  may  become  uncomfortable 
during  the  test,  and  if  you  wish  to  stop  at  any  time,  please 


. 


15 


feel  free  to  do  so.  Any  questions?  OK,  here  is  your 
first  picture. 

The  fourteen  cards  were  scrambled  in  the  order  shown  in  the 
Appendix  to  conceal  the  pairing  between  the  two  sets  of  seven. 

I  discovered  after  the  test  that  the  subject  was  not  only  unaware 
of  the  corresponding  sets,  but  also  unaware  that  the  study  dealt 
with  some  aspect  of  race.  Nevertheless,  he  said  he  was  conscious, 
at  times,  of  the  race  of  the  figures. 

The  method  of  analysis  will  be  discussed  in  the  next  section, 

A  question  may  be  raised  about  the  sense  in  which  the  two  sets  are 
similar.  It  is  true  that  the  difference  between  any  two  correspond¬ 
ing  cards  will  be  greater  than  a  mere  racial  difference.  For  ex¬ 
ample,  card  five  shows  a  young  black  male  sitting  with  a  letter  in 
hand  while  number  eleven  shows  a  young  white  male  sitting  with  a 
telephone  receiver  in  hand.  Such  differences  are  unimportant  for 
this  study  for  several  reasons.  First,  the  age  and  sex  of  the 
figures  is  identifiably  similar.  Second,  the  pictures  are  in¬ 
tentionally  out  of  focus  and  highly  ambiguous.  The  letter  in  pic¬ 
ture  five,  for  example,  was  perceived  as  a  book.  Third,  our  in¬ 
terest  is  primarily  in  the  symbolic  form  in  which  very  general 
types  of  ideas  are  embodied,  and  not  in  the  subtleties  of  content. 
For  this  reason  we  tape  recorded  the  subject 9 s  speech. 

Let9s  now  examine  the  impact  of  racial  forms  on  the  thought 


of  one  person. 


. 


16 


II 

The  Impact  of  Black  Human  Forms 

There  are  many  ways  to  analyze  a  TAT  protocol.  At  one  end  of 
a  range  there  is  TAT  analysis  done  in  conjunction  with  clinical 
work.  Here  the  interest  is  in  the  particular  woven  texture  of  ideas 
which  unknown  to  the  patient  have  fed  his  suffering.  The  TAT  is 
used  diagnostically.  At  the  other  end  lie  those  research  studies 
in  which  a  statistically  significant  variation  Is  sought  in  the 
frequency  of  particular  words  or  phrases*  pre-selected  on  the 
assumption  that  their  meaning  will  be  the  same  for  different  people. 
The  TAT  is  used  almost  as  a  questionnaire . 

The  method  we  will  use  owes  something  to  both  these  techniques. 
The  writers  whose  work  was  reviewed  are  all  convinced  that  the 
strange  and  racist  fantasies  they  study  owe  their  formation  to 
human  passions  and  fears.  In  part  I  share  this  belief  since  I 
have  witnessed  such  processes  in  my  own  mind  and  since  psychoanal¬ 
ysis  has  shown  that  people  are  motivated  to  distort  the  humanness 
of  others  even  though  they  sometimes  know  better.  But  since  we 
are  wondering  about  a  general  impact  of  the  black  on  the  white 
mind,  it  seems  sensible  to  confine  our  investigation  of  the  affec¬ 
tive  significance  of  any  story  to  some  general  properties  of  con¬ 
flict  and  defense. 

In  the  next  section  we  will  define  the  notions  of  "conflict" 
and  "defense"  more  clearly.  Let  us  state  here  that  we  are  not 
using  the  term  "defense"  in  the  traditional  psychodynamic  sense. 

That  is,  for  the  purposes  of  this  paper  a  defense  is  not  a  mech- 


» 


17 


anism  employed  to  reduce  the  signal  anxiety  associated  with  intra¬ 
psychic  conflict.  Rather  we  are  considering  "defense"  to  he  an 
intellectual  operation  upon  intellectual  ideas  of  "wishes,"  which 
structure  a  relationship  of  union  between  objects,  and  upon  intel¬ 
lectual  ideas  of  "fears,"  which  structure  a  separation  of  objects. 

We  assume  that  such  ideas  often  have  affective  importance  and  are 
the  elements  of  conflict,  A  defensive  operation  is  successful  in¬ 
sofar  as  it  fulfills  a  wish  or  mitigates  a  loss. 

In  our  analysis  we  will  look  at  whether  the  manifest  story 
deals  more  with  wishes  or  with  fears.  This  will  not  tell  us  much 
about  defense,  however,  A  story  which  expresses  a  threatening 
separation  may  serve  to  make  loss  tolerable.  In  therapy,  a  patient 
may  defend  against  his  fear  of  abandonment  with  an  "i'll  leave  you 
before  you  leave  me'' fantasy.  Therefore,  we  distinguish  between 
surface  structure  and  deep  structure. 

Surface  structure  is  the  particular  system  of  relationships 
between  the  figures  of  the  manifest  story.  It  represents  one 
particular  arrangement  of  a  set  of  underlying  themes --the  deep 
structure.  We  will  identify  these  underlying  themes  by  a  compari¬ 
son  of  the  manifest  themes  of  each  pair  of  stories.  Since  we  will 
pay  particular  attention  to  union  and  separation  the  "relative 
def ensiveness "  of  each  member  of  the  pair  may  be  discerned.  This 
method  is  not,  strictly  speaking,  an  analysis  of  psychodynamics . 

Deep  structure  is  analagous ,  but  not  identical  with  unconscious 
conflict,  and  we  do  not  assume,  therefore,  that  the  subject  actu¬ 
ally  experiences  the  conflicts  we  will  identify.  In  fact,  there 
are  innumerable  layers  of  wishes  and  fears.  Our  very  general  in¬ 
terpretations  represent  simply  one  way  of  conceptualizing  structures 


18 


which  are  consistent  with  the  stories. 

In  addition  to  this  thematic  approach  we  will  be  inspecting 
the  organization  of  the  speech  and  the  use  of  particular  words  or 
phrases.  Our  hunch  is  that  in  order  to  investigate  the  questions 
raised  in  the  previous  section  we  will  reasonably  have  to  ask  with 
what  kind  of  construct ions  we  are  dealing, 

"Thought,”  from  the  point  of  view  we  are  taking,  is  an  active 
process  and  is  distinguished  from  "thoughts"  or  "ideas"  which  are 
structures.  Ideas  can  be  shaped  by  external  patterns  created  inde¬ 
pendently  of  the  mind’s  activity  (thought  in  accommodation).  And 
the  mind  can  reorganize  these  external  patterns  and  combine  them 
with  others  in  structured  ways  (thought  in  assimilation).  Both 
processes  can  occur  together  (thought  in  equilibrium).  These  two 
functional  activities  transform  thoughts  and  can  be  performed  by 
any  person  capable  of  thought.  As  I  write  this  paper,  for  example, 
daydreams  of  distant  people  and  unspoken  words  frequently  dovetail 
with  activities  conforming  to  the  requirements  which  any  medical 
school  thesis  must  obey.  The  domination  of  either  of  these  poles 
of  activity  over  the  other  results  in  two  relatively  different  pro¬ 
ducts,  identified  by  their  more  or  less  "dreamlike"  properties. 

The  notion  of  "dreamlike"  properties  refers  to  the  manifest 
properties  of  a  symbolic  product.  It  is  an  imprecise  notion,  and 
strictly  speaking  it  says  nothing  about  either  thought  processes 
or  thoughts.  It  is  suggestive,  however,  of  the  relative  difference 
between  the  results  of  assimilation  and  accommodation.  This  relative 
difference,  as  intuitively  obvious  as  that  between  dreams  and  imi¬ 
tation,  defines  a  continuum  on  which  the  TAT  stories  lie,  A  more 
precise  discussion  of  the  creative  processes  themselves  will  be 


19 

postponed  until  the  next  section,  after  we  have  examined  several 
stories  in  terms  of  this  continuum* 

In  order  to  clarify  this  notion  of  dreamlike  properties  let’s 
simply  list  some  of  the  qualities  of  manifest  dreams. 

Dreams  seem  different  from  waking  life.  The  dreamer  pays 
little  heed  to  the  contextual  relations  of  the  real  world.  Unreal 
and  strange  things  happen.  The  narrative  sequence  Is  fragile  and 
subject  to  disruption  and  irrational  leaps.  Events  may  be  repeated 
over  and  over,  or  sustained  long  after  they  should  have  been  com¬ 
pleted,  It  may  be  hard  to  know  just  what  is  going  on  in  this  world 
of  now  absurd,  now  mundane  events. 

The  sense  of  temporal  context  is  different  in  dreams.  In 
waking  life  one  can  conceive  of  past,  present,  and  future  actions. 
In  dreams,  we  draw  upon  the  past  for  material  but  exist  only  with¬ 
in  present  time.  There  is  no  expression  of  condit ionality ,  neces¬ 
sity,  or  wish  in  manifest  dreams.  Things  just  happen  or  they  are. 
The  expression  is  all  in  the  indicative  mood.  Cause  and  effect 
cannot  be  expressed  directly.  There  is  no  way  to  represent  an 
action  which  has  not  yet  occurred,  and  the  events  of  a  dream  se¬ 
quence  are  unimportant  as  soon  as  they  cease  to  be  immediately  pre¬ 
sent  , 

The  sense  of  spatial  context  is  disrupted  in  dreams.  An  ob¬ 
ject  may  appear  out  of  its  normal  place.  The  scene  of  dreams  can 
shift  from  place  to  place  with  no  explicit  transition.  Objects 
can  change  size  and  shape. 

Dreams  do  preserve  some  sense  of  space,  however,  since  they 
are  visual.  Present  objects  can  be  compared  in  terms  of  dimension, 
shape,  and  color,  but  not  in  terms  of  intangible  qualities. 


•  J c 


Contradict ion  does  not  exist  in  dreams.  Opposites  can  ex¬ 
ist  side  by  side  without  conflict.  People  in  dreams  may  possess 
rationally  contradictory  qualities  without  a  sense  of  incompati¬ 
bility,  Internal  conflict,  therefore,  does  not  exist  in  dream  fig¬ 
ures,  Although  the  dreamer  may  have  feelings  and  thoughts  in  his 
dream,  the  dream  figures  never  self-ref lect ,  think,  or  feel,  though 
they  may  speak.  They  simply  act,  or  exist. 

People  who  are  accustomed  to  thinking  systematically  about 
language  may  question  the  validity  of  drawing  inferences  about 
thought  processes  on  the  basis  of  linguistic  features,  mainly  se¬ 
mantics  and  syntactics.  Our  analysis,  let  me  say  at  this  point, 
assumes  a  distinction  between  what  is  called  linguistic  "competence, 
the  knowledge  of  the  rules  of  a  language,  and  linguistic  "perfor¬ 
mance,"  the  ways  in  which  these  rules  are  used  and  violated.  Since 
our  subject’s  knowledge  of  English  probably  did  not  change  during 
the  test  in  response  to  my  grunts,  nods,  simple  questions,  and 
silence,  we  can  view  our  analysis  as  an  investigation  into  his  per¬ 
formance,  This  performance,  the  variable  use  of  linguistic  compe¬ 
tence,  can  be  viewed  as  changing  under  the  impact  of  the  variable 
activity  of  the  subject’s  thought. 

The  subject  and  I  compared  notes  after  the  test.  He  was  sur¬ 
prised  to  find  out  the  experiment  dealt  with  race,  assuming  that  I 
was  interested  in  the  "male-female  type"  themes  which  abound  in 
his  stories.  Like  many  theorists  of  race  symbolization,  he  had 
difficulty  imagining  that  one  could  be  interested  in  the  forms  he 
used  to  embody  particular  themes,  I  admitted  to  a  lingering  doubt 
in  my  own  head  at  the  time,  and  stated  the  experiment  was  based 
upon  unformulated  hypotheses,  or,  more  bluntly,  a  guess.  Of  the 


,  • 


21 


several  cards  of  which  the  subject  said  "it  struck  me,"  one  was  of 
"the  guy  and  girl  holding  hands,"  Let’s  start,  then,  with  the  pair 
of  which  this  story  is  a  member. 

Picture  number  nine:  A  young  white  woman  with  lowered  eyes 
is  turned  slightly  away  from  a  young  white  man  who  is  looking  at 
her  and  has  his  hands  on  her  shoulders. 

Story  number  nine:-®-  (long  pause)  The  guy  here  has  found  out  1 
urn  that  this  girl  doesn’t  care  for  him  as  much  as  uh  he  thought,  2 
possibly  as  much  as  she  let  him  think,  either  consciously  or  un-  3 
consciously,  and  uh  he’s  upset.  It  doesn’t,  it  doesn’t  involve,  4 
say,  her  sleeping  with  another  guy  or  something  like  that.  It’s  5 
just  that  uh  she  just  doesn’t  give  him  as  much  as  he  wants  and  as  6 

much  as  he  feels  he  gives  her.  And  so  (he)  is  in  a  position  to  7 

be  asked,  is  in  a  position  of  asking  her  to  either— give  more,  8 

treat  him  as  he  wants  to  be  treated,  as  he  feels  their,  you  know,  9 
to  in  other  words,  to  get  in  deeper,  to  uh  let  the  relationship  go  10 
as  far  as  he  thinks  it  could  go.  And  she  is,  very  indecisive,  be-  11 
cause  she  she  doesn’t  know  where  she  stands  as  far  with  him.  She  12 
doesn’t  know  what  she  wants  out  of  it  urn.  She’s  got  other  commit-  13 
ments,  not  romantic  type  commitments,  but  she  has  other  friends  14 

that  she  doesn’t  want  to  be,  doesn’t  want  to  have  to  stop  seeing  15 

because  she’s  going  to  devote  everything  to  him,  uh,  and  so,  she  16 
has  to  turn  away.  She  can’t  look  at  him  knowing  how  much  he  needs  17 
an  answer,  because  she  doesn’t  have  it  yet.  She  needs  time.  So  18 
she  will  just  turn  away  and  sort  of  walk  off  and— they’ll  separate  19 
uh  this  time,  you  know,  before  deciding  anything,  and  have  to  20 

1 

Additions  and  nonverbal  behavior  are  contained  in  brackets,  (  )a 

Sentences  are  marked  off  intuitively. 


22 


come  back  together.  It’s  not  going  to  be  resolved ,  uh,  this  21 

afternoon,  and  so  its  she’s  going  to  have  to  come  back  to  him  or  22 
he's  going  to  have  to  come  back  to  her  after  a  very  short  period  23 
of  time,  after  she’s  gotten  things  straightened  out  in  her  own  24 
head,  2 5 

Picture  number  two:  A  young  black  woman  with  lowered  eyes  is 
turned  slightly  away  from  a  young  white  man  who  is  looking  at  her. 
At  the  bottom  of  the  picture  a  part  of  his  hand,  which  is  holding 
her  wrist,  is  visible. 

Story  number  two:  (long  pause).  It  looks  like  there's  been  1 
urn  like  the  girl  has  had  urn  some  sort  of  a  tough  time,  got  some 
bad  news  or  something,  and  come  together  with  this  guy,  who,  she  3 
knows  fairly  well.  They  know  each  other,  and  they’re  fairly  4 

close,  close  enough  to  be  touching,  and  she’s  looking  for  uh,  5 

someone  to  talk  to,  someone  to  sympathize  with  her.  It’s  not,  it  6 

hasn’t  been  a  fight  between  them.  But  it  might  be,  she  can  be  7 

coming  with,  something  she’s  thought  about  their  relationship,  or  8 
she’s  coming  with,  uh--something  that’s  happened  to  her  directly  9 
that  didn’t  involve  him  at  all,  uh,  something  bad  to  another  10 
friend,  or  her  mother  died,  or  she  got  wiped  out  on  a  test,  that  11 
sort  of  thing.  They’re  just  sort  of  really  nonverbally,  because  12 
their  their  relationship  is  beyond  that  really.  They  don’t  have  13 
to  talk  just  to  be  together.  It  comforts  her.  And  uh  from  here  14 
they’ll  go  out  and,  not  out  necessarily,  but  they’ll  go  on,  and  15 
on  a  very  sort  of  calm  level  uh  and  be  together  for  a  while  uh  16 
not  necessarily  doing  anything  very  exciting,  uh  (It’s)  just  17 

their  being  together  that  matters  to  uh  both  of  them.  And  so  I  18 
I  just  see  them  having  a  you  know,  either  where  they  are  or  19 


8  1  r 


23 


someplace  to  eat  or  something,  but  just  talking  quietly  in  the  20 
next  couple  hours  and  sort  of  resting  on  each  other,  particu-  21 

larly  her,  you  know,  on  the  security  that  he’s  providing  after 
the  tough  time  she’s  had.  23 

A  preliminary  comment  on  these  stories  in  terms  of  the  liter¬ 
ature  is  in  order.  It  is  obvious  that  although  both  stories  may 
have  forbidden  thoughts  lingering  underneath,  the  notion  of  the 
"projection  of  id  impulses"  is  useless  in  understanding  the  changes 
between  the  two  stories.  As  far  as  the  manifest  story  is  concern¬ 
ed,  the  only  characters  who  are  In  any  sense  "bad"  are  the  pair  in 
story  number  nine,  particularly  "the  girl"  who  is  leaving  the  guy, 
the  central  figure  in  the  story.  Sexuality  is  specifically  mention¬ 
ed  only  in  the  story  with  white  figures:  "it  doesn’t  involve,  say, 
her  sleeping  with  another  guy  or  something  like  that."  She’s  got 
"other  commitments,  not-~romant ic  type  commitments,"  Further,  the 
references  to  giving,  to  getting  in  deeper,  to  letting  the  relation¬ 
ship  go  as  far  as  he  thinks  it  could  go,  are  more  suggestive  of 
sexual  themes  than  the  "not  necessarily  doing  anything  very  excit¬ 
ing"  and  the  reference  to  "close  enough  to  be  touching"  of  story 
two.  "The  girl"  of  story  two  is  a  victim  who  needs  and  receives 
comfort.  She  is  hardly  the  stupid,  dirty,  evil,  harmful,  dominated, 
avoided,  promiscuous,  et  cetera  object  of  the  "universal  racism" 
which  so  many  writers  have  condemned.  This  is  not  to  say  that  the 
impact  of  race  does  not  enter  the  picture,  nor  that  such  fantasies 
as  number  two  do  not  in  some  way  contribute  to  racism.  To  under¬ 
stand  this,  however,  we  need  to  develop  notions  which  are  at  least 
capable  of  encompassing  the  manifest  story  without  distorting  it 
beyond  recognition. 


24 


The  major  themes  of  the  two  stories  are  roughly  similar,,  yet 
they  appear  quite  different  because  of  a  difference  in  their  organ¬ 
ization,  Both  stories  deal  with  one  person  in  need  of  another. 

Both  depict  a  response  by  the  other  to  that  need.  Both  contain  an 
idea  of  loss,  separation,  or  a  sustained  hurt.  Both  suggest  a  re¬ 
lationship  between  a  woman  and  people  or  events  outside  of  her  in¬ 
volvement  with  the  guy.  And  both  suggest  that  there  is  an  impact 
of  the  outside  relationship  on  the  primary  one. 

The  striking  differences  lie  in  the  organization  of  these 
themes.  In  story  number  two,  the  woman  is  in  need  and  the  man  re¬ 
sponds  positively.  In  nine  the  man  is  in  need  and  the  woman  re¬ 
sponds  negatively.  In  two  the  woman  sustains  the  hurt  in  an  outside 
event,  while  in  nine  the  man  sustains  the  hurt  in  his  relationship 
with  the  girl.  In  two  the  woman’s  negative  relationship  to  the 
outside  brings  the  two  together.  In  nine,  her  positive  relation¬ 
ship  to  the  outside  contributes  to  keeping  them  apart.  The  two 
stories  can  be  thought  of  as  mirror  images  of  one  another,  each 
preserving  an  opposition  between  inside  and  outside,  each  express¬ 
ing  a  loss  of  and  preservation  of  some  relationship.  Thematically 
they  are  transformations  of  one  another  or  of  some  underlying  struc¬ 
ture  , 

The  result  is  that  story  two  represents  primarily  the  fulfill¬ 
ment  of  a  wish  and  the  continuance  of  a  relationship  characterized 
by  friendship,  closeness,  and  security  "beyond"  words.  Number  nine, 
by  contrast,  represents  primarily  a  loss,  a  separation,  a  turning 
away.  By  removing  the  notion  of  loss  from  the  level  of  the  inter¬ 
action  between  the  two  actors  in  number  nine,  to  the  level  of  the 
woman’s  relationship  with  some  other  event  or  object  in  number  two, 


25 


the  subject  has  masterfully  turned  story  nine  inside  out.  Although 
the  loss  and  the  need  still  relate  to  a  single  individual ,  as  in 
nine,  the  subject  has  been  able  to  preserve  the  idea  and  permit 
gratification  of  the  need  at  the  same  time.  In  other  words ,  fantasy 
number  two  has  the  charaterist ics  of  a  defensive  fantasy,  including 
compromise  formation,  relative  to  story  nine,  which  also  has  its 
defensive  aspects. 

It  should  be  clear  that  closeness  and  unity  pervade  story 
number  two,  while  number  nine  chronicles  the-trouble-I ’ m-having- 
with-my-girl .  The  multiple  use  of  "they51  and  "each  other"  replaces 
the  "he  and  she"  of  nine.  In  nine,  the  only  "they"  is  used  in  the 
phrase  "they’ll  separate."  The  only  "their"  stands  as  an  adjective 
with  nothing  to  modify  and  the  phrase  of  which  it  is  a  part  is 
dropped  and  replaced. 

We  should  also  note  that  the  subject,  in  number  two,  seems  very 
unconcerned  with  the  characteristics  of  the  black  image  on  the  TAT 
card.  Her  two  most  obvious  properties,  blackness  and  womanness, 
are  not  manifestly  used  in  this  story.  Although  she  is  referred 
to  as  "she"  occasionally ,  none  o^  the  words  used  to  describe  her 
are  as  suggestive  of  her  biological  sex  as  the  "her  sleeping  with 
another  guy"  of  story  nine,  (Though  a  man,  the  subject  has  little 
trouble  identifying  with  her.  In  projective  test  terminology,  she 
corresponds  most  closely  to  the  "hero,")  Further,  the  context  of 
her  life  is  obscure.  As  far  as  the  subject  is  concerned  the  death 
of  her  mother  and  a  bad  experience  with  a  test  are  both  "that  sort 
of  thing,,"  Although  there  is  a  sense  of  her  outside  world,  it’s 
nature  is  opaque  to  the  subject.  Her  fundamental  humanness,  her 
need,  her  hurt,  her  looking  for  someone  to  talk  to,  her  experience 


26 


of  comfort  and  security,  prevail.  In  a  way,  she  is  used  as  a  form¬ 
less  pan-human  being  seeking  and  finding  refuge  in  closeness.  Her 
companion,  of  course,  is  even  more  devoid  of  particularity.  He  re¬ 
mains  a  simple  human  presence  from  beginning  to  end. 

Use  of  the  forms  on  the  cards  in  this  non-contextual  way  suggests 
a  more  dreamlike  construction  of  story  two  as  opposed  to  number 
nine.  Keeping  the  manifest  properties  of  dreams  in  mind,  let’s 
check  on  those  aspects  of  speech  which  might  reveal  these  proper¬ 
ties  » 

A  greater  sense  of  uncertainty  is  conveyed  in  story  two  by  the 
use  of  three  expressions:  "some”  as  in  "some  sort  of,"  used  nine 
times;  ’'really"  used  twice;  and  "you  know"  used  twice.  By  con¬ 
trast,  in  story  nine  "some"  is  used  once,  "you  know"  once,  and 
"really"  not  at  all. 

Notions  of  cause  and  effect  are  expressed  six  times  in  nine, 
including  the  "and  so’s"  which  are  followed  by  consequences ,  and 
only  one  in  story  two,  following  an  adverb  in  an  ungrammatical  way. 

The  conditional  phrase  used  in  two  Is  an  expression  of  the  subject’s 
attitude  toward  the  story,  but  not  part  of  the  story  itself.  In 
nine,  a  conditional  phrase  is  used  once  in  a  similar  sense,  and 
once  as  part  of  the  story  (line  11).  The  imperative  mood  Is  used 
once  in  story  two  (line  14) ,  but  negatively,  and  six  times  in  nine 
(lines  15,  1 6 ,  17,  21,  22,  23).  The  optative  mood  is  never  used 
in  two,  but  is  used  four  times  in  number  nine  (lines  6,  8,  13,  and 
15) . 

There  are  four  references  to  time  in  number  nine  (lines  18, 

20,  22,  24),  and  two  in  two  (lines  16,  21).  References  to  future 
actions  are  five  in  story  two  (lines  15,  15,  16,  20,  21),  and  five 


27 


in  nine  (lines  15,  19,  21,  22,  23),  References  to  past  action  are 
two  in  nine  and  ten  in  story  two. 

Contradictions  of  a  previous  idea  occur  three  times  in  two 
(lines  9,  14,  19,  20),  and  not  at  all  in  number  nine.  Phrases  of 
comparison  of  intangible  qualities  are  used  twice  in  number  two 
(lines  13,  14,  22),  and  eleven  times  in  nine  (lines  2,  2,  6,  6,  8, 

9,  10,  11,  1?,  18,  19,  20,  22,  23), 

A  phrase  suggesting  reflective  thought  occurs  once  in  story 
two  (line  8),  but  is  quickly  denied.  In  nine,  such  phrases  occur 
nine  times  (lines  2,  7,  9,  10,  11,  12,  13,  17,  20,  24).  References 
to  people  feeling  in  number  two  are  all  in  terms  of  actions,  events, 
and  consequences  of  actions*  The  girl  doesn3t  explicitly  feel  bad, 
but  she  has  had  a  "tough  time,"  or  got  some  "bad  news."  Explicit 
references  to  feelings  occur  three  times  in  number  nine  (lines  3» 

7,  9 ) » 

In  transcribing  from  tape,  it  was  necessary  to  arbitrarily 
and  intuitively  mark  off  sentences.  I  chose  to  mark  them  in  such 
a  way  that  each  phrase  has  at  least  one  noun  phrase  and  one  verb 
phrase,  and  consider  phrases  the  subject  corrects  and  phrases  which 
disrupt  grammaticalness  as  not  being  part  of  the  sentence.  One 
could  argue  about  this  highly  primitive  method  and  the  particulars 
of  grammaticalness.  Nevertheless,  by  this  method  the  ratio  of 
words  of  four  or  more  letters  which  are  part  of  a  grammatical  sen¬ 
tence  to  words  which  are  not  part  of  a  sentence  is  about  3  to  1  in 
story  two.  The  ratio  in  story  nine  is  about  6.5  to  1. 

None  of  these  differences  are  statistically  significant  since 
the  total  number  of  items  is  so  small*  But  in  every  case  but  one 
they  suggest  a  drift  toward  dreamlike  thought  in  story  number  two. 


28 


(The  one  exception  is  the  equal  number  of  references  to  future  action 
in  the  two  stories,)  Number  nine  uses  more  integrated,  organized, 
discursive,  relational,  and  complex  speech.  And  just  as  dreams  so 
often  flee  the  memory  when  we  awake,  the  subject  seems  distant  from 
the  unknown  details  of  the  guy  and  girl  holding  hands.  He  says  "it 
looks  like,  it  might  be,  she  can  be,  I  just  see  them,51  The  trouble 
between  the  guy  and  the  girl,  however,  has  the  immediacy  of  a  real 
situation.  The  subject  is  still  aware  of  painting  a  picture,  but 
he  fills  the  canvas  with  ease.  Chunks  of  social  reality  are  used 
with  assurance.  The  outline  of  the  story  is  clear  from  the  initial 
declaration  that  "the  guy  here  has  found  out  um  that  this  girl 
doesn3t  care  for  him  as  much  as  uh  he  thought,"  A  story  seems  to 
suggest  itself  to  the  subject.  Still,  he  volunteers  that  "the  one 
with  the  guy  and  girl  holding  hands  struck  me,"  picture  number  two. 
Perhaps  we  could  say  that  the  subject  was  close  to  the  meaning  of 
a  story  of  unknown  details,  while  in  nine  he  was  distant  from  the 
meaning  of  a  story  with  known  details. 

Story  number  two,  then,  is  more  defensive,  more  expressive  of 
union,  and  more  dreamlike  than  number  nine.  In  addition,  the  black 
figure  is  used  primarily  for  its  personal  human  meaning,  with  little 
use  made  of  its  form.  Let3s  now  examine  two  stories  in  which  the 
impact  of  race  seems  strikingly  different,  but  which  was  similarly 
flagged  by  the  subject  after  the  test. 

Picture  number  three:  Two  young  white  men  are  looking  at  each 
other.  One  has  his  hand  on  the  door  of  a  phone  booth,  and  the  other 

is  several  feet  away,  facing  the  first.  It  is  night. 

1 

Story  number  three:  (long  wait).  These  two  guys  have  had  a 

2 

disagreement  um  and  the  one  is  stepping  into  the  phone  booth.  It's 


29 


3  4 

a  very  decisive  sort  of  a  thing.  And  I’m  not  sure.  I  can’t  tell 

5  6 

what  it’s  about.  But  He’s  taking  some  action,  that  uh  He’s  calling 

someone,  perhaps  a  girl  that  they  both  know  uh  after  disagreeing 

with  the  other  guy,  almost  as  a  proof  of  of  his  superiority  or  uh 

his  (proof)  that  he’s  going  to  to  it  despite  what  the  other  guy 

7 

things,  uh  The  other  guy’s  obviously  very  upset,  and  he’s  un  As 

8 

soon  as  the  guy  gets  into  the  phone  booth,  they’re  right  now  making 

their  uh  sort  of  glaring  at  each  other,  in  a  way  of  saying  goodbye. 

9 

uh  The  one’s  going  to  go  in  the  phone  booth  and  make  his  call  and 
the  other’s  going  to  leave,  after  this  final  conf rontation. 

D.B.:  What  does  the  guy  who’s  going  into  the  phone  booth  feel? 
Subject:  He-regrets  having  to  do  what  he’s  doing  because  he  seems 
to  v- value  his  relationship  with  the  other  guy.  He’s  they  their 
they  They've  been  friends  and  something  uh  I’ll  say  a  girl,  has 
come  between  them  and  he  is  sorry  it  has  to  be  the  way  it  has  to 
be.  and  uh  he’s  and  he  doesn’t  He’s  sort  of  uh  upset  that  things 
have  to  be  a  certain  way,  (and)  that  something  is  being  forced  on 
him  that  he  would  rather  not  have.  But  he’s  going  to  go  through 
with  with  with  the  phone  call.  He’s  going  to  go  through  with  it  and 
um--sacrif ice  his  friendship  with  the  guy  for  urn  the  object  of  the 
phone  call. 

Picture  number  eight:  A  young  white  man  and  a  young  black  man 
are  looking  at  each  other.  The  white  stands  with  his  hand  on  a 
half-opened  car  door.  The  black  is  gesturing  toward  him  from  sev¬ 
eral  feet  away.  It  is  night. 

1 

Story  number  eight:  (pause,  sits  forward).  Outside  of  a  movie 

2 

theater,  or  some  some  such  place.  A  white  guy  has  come  out  uh  ob- 

3  '  4 

viously  with  money.  He’s  got  rather  fancy  clothes  on.  The  bla,ck 


. 

I 

■  ..  > 

.  ! 


30 


guy  comes  up  uh  has  come  up  to  spare  change  him,  and  knowing  he  can 

play  on  the  other  guy's  white  guilt,  sort  of  that  kind  of  feeling 

urn  knowing  his  advantage,  and  that  it's  it's  increasingly  hard  for 

a  white  guy  to  turn  down  uh  a  poor  looking  black,  and  so  he  comes 
5  6 
up.  and  The  white  is  feeling  very  threatened,  he's  un  Although 

he's  uh  enlightened  (smile)  in  his  racial  views,  he  recognizes  that 

the  crime  and  uh  that  sort  of  thing  that  he  reads  about  all  the 

time,  and  therefore  he's  scared  that  this  guy  uh  uh  is  wants  more 

than  just  his  spare  change,  that  he  might  do  him  some  harm,  uh 

7 

Meanwhile,  the  black  guy  knows,  pretty  much  knows,  what  goes  through 

the  white  guy's  mind  and  uses  it  hoping  that  the  guy  will  give  him 

8 

some  spare  change  and  uh  take  off.  and  But  rather  than  do  this,  the 

9 

white  guy  has  his— security »  He's  got  his  car  he  can  leap  into  and 

uh  I  think  he's  got— a  girl  in  the  car  already  who  he  feels  respon- 

10 

sible  for,  and  It  sort  of  doubles  his  fear  but  it  also  doubles  his 

11 

courage,  and  he  just  son't  He's  just  going  to  say  no  and  get  in 
the  car  and  leave,  urn  the  black  guy  staying  around  to  try  urn  the 
next  person  that  comes  along. 

This  pair  of  stories  presents  an  example  of  a  racist  transfor¬ 
mation,  according  to  one  of  the  criteria  presented  in  the  last  sec¬ 
tion,  In  story  number  eight,  the  avoidance  behavior  of  the  white 
is  based  on  a  belief  about  the  likely  Intentions  of  a  black  man. 

The  white  reads  the  newspapers  and  knows  that  blacks  commit  crime, 
and  therefore  he  feels  threatened, 

V/e  could  hardly  understand  this  story  by  reference  to  some 
automatic  mechanism  of  projection  onto  blacks  or  by  labeling  our 
subject  an  "aversive  racist,"  To  begin  with,  only  particular 
elements  of  the  standard  collection  of  racist  beliefs  are  selected. 


,  • 

,  • 


31 


Explicit  ideas  of  domination,  superiority,  and  sexual  aggressive¬ 
ness  are  absent.  The  black  may  be  threatening,  but  he  is  smart. 

More  importantly,  our  subject  produced  some  nonracist  stories  with 
black  figures.  Story  two  is  one  example,  and  may  suggest  that  the 
sex  of  the  black  figure  is  the  significant  factor  for  this  subject. 
This  conclusion  is  invalid,  however,  as  evidenced  by  story  number 
twelve  (see  Appendix).  In  twelve,  a  white  man  and  a  black  man  are 
"friends”  and  "colleagues"  who  "know  each  other."  The  black  man 
"refreshes"  the  white  who  is  "resting  after  delivering  a  lecture," 
and  "both  go  on  to  finish  the  day  in  good  spirits."  But  finally, 

I  think  our  subject  would  be  embarrassed  to  reread  story  eight.  He 
seemed  to  sense  its  racist  elements  even  in  the  telling,  as  his 
apologetic  smile  in  the  middle  of  the  story  suggests.  The  story, 
in  fact,  is  unsettling  as  much  for  its  bizarre  conventionality  as 
for  its  racism.  It’s  hard  to  dismiss  it  as  the  probabilistic 
eruption  of  man’s  evil  nature.  But  we  can  wonder  how  the  mind  uses 
the  image  of  the  black  in  ways  as  disparate  as  stories  eight  and 
two. 

If  we  look  into  the  themes  of  three  and  eight  we  find,  once 
again,  a  similarity.  In  both  stories  two  guys  are  in  conflict. 

One  guy,  the  central  figure  in  each  story,  is  leaving  the  other  guy. 
The  other  guy  doesn’t  want  him  to  leave,  at  least  not  yet.  The  guy 
who’s  leaving  is  ambivalent.  In  number  eight  it  Is  hard  to  turn 
down  a  poor  looking  black,  while  in  three  it  is  a  question  of  friend 
ship.  Finally,  the  guy  who’s  leaving  has  a  girl  waiting  for  him. 

These  similarities  in  the  fantasied  relations,  and  the  discom¬ 
fort  of  the  hero  in  both  stories  permit  us  to  view  this  pair  as  two 
different  ways  of  solving  one  problem:  the  inherent  contradict ion 


32 

in  sacrificing,  or  leaving  a  friend.  In  the  TAT  pictures,  two  men 
are  together.  Soon,  our  subject  says,  they  will  be  apart.  If  it's 
friendship,  why  the  sacrifice? 

There  are,  no  doubt,  layers  of  related  problems  underlying  this 
one,  but  the  idea  of  leaving  a  friend  is  one  way  of  identifying  a 
deep  structure  of  conflict  which  could  surely  be  transformed  into 
these  two  manifest  stories.  There  are  two  primary  ways  to  mitigate 
this  sense  of  intentional  loss.  One  is  to  make  it  appear  as  if 
leaving  is  being  dictated  by  some  outside  force:  it  is  not  inten¬ 
tional,  The  other  is  to  turn  the  friend  into  something  else:  it 
is  not  a  loss.  In  the  second  part  of  story  three  (after  my  ques¬ 
tion),  the  first  method  is  employed,  "They’ve  been  friends,  and 
something,  I’ll  say  a  girl,  has  come  between  them.., is  being  forced 
on  him  that  he  would  rather  not  have,"  In  the  first  part  of  the 
story  the  second  method  is  used.  The  hero  leaves  the  other  decisive*™ 
ly,  while  whatever  friendship  exists  between  them  is  not  mentioned. 
Their  relationship  is  characterized  by  glaring  conf rontation  and 
disagreement  over  superiority,  independence ,  and  a  girl. 

Both  methods  are  employed  in  eight.  The  hero  is  not  only 
forced  to  leave  but  forced  to  "leap,"  And  the  motivating  agent  is 
no  longer  obscure:  it  is  the  friend  of  the  last  part  of  story 
three,  who  has  now  become  a  threatening  black  man.  The  self-willed 
loss  becomes  an  achieved  escape.  The  reward,  in  three  "I’ll  say  a 
girl"  or  "the  object  of  the  phone  call,"  has  become  security,  a 
car,  and  the  responsibility  of  a  girl. 

Like  story  number  two,  this  transf ormat ion  is  defensive.  The 
manifest  story,  however,  emphasizes  the  avoidance  of  a  loss  rather 
than  realization  of  a  union.  At  the  same  time,  the  separateness  of 


33 


the  two  men  is  almost  organic*  No  words  are  spoken.  One  is  rich, 
one  is  poor.  One  is  named  "black  guy,"  one  is  named  "white  guy." 
Neither  knows  the  other’s  intentions.  Suspicion  prevails.  They  are 
strangers.  There  is  none  of  the  mutuality,  the  glaring  at  each 
other,  the  disagreement,  the  saying  goodbye,  the  valued  friendship 
that  we  see  between  the  guys  at  the  phone  booth, 

A  glance  at  the  use  of  language  in  this  pair  of  stories  leaves 
the  impression  of  a  less  dreamlike,  more  vigorously  textured,  in¬ 
terracial  fantasy.  We  may  check  this  impression  by  tabulating 
speech  elements  of  the  same  categories  which  differentiate  two  and 
nine.  Thirteen  categories  have  been  defined.  Larger  totals  in  the 
first  three  of  these  suggest  more  dreamlike  assimilatory  thought. 
Larger  totals  in  the  remaining  ten  categories  suggest  more  accommo¬ 
dative  thought.  The  categories  are  as  follows: 

1.  Phrases  suggesting  a  conditional  attitude  of  the  subject  toward 
his  story, 

2.  References  to  past  action. 

3.  Ideas  which  contradict  previous  ideas, 

4.  Cause  and  effect  constructions , 

5.  Phrases  expressing  conditional  relationships  within  the  story. 

6.  Verb  phrases  expressing  necessity, 

7.  Verb  phrases  expressing  a  wish. 

8.  References  to  time. 

9.  References  to  future  action, 

10.  Comparisons  of  intangible  qualities, 

11.  References  to  reflective  thought, 

12.  References  to  feelings, 

13 o  The  ratio  of  words  of  four  or  more  letters  which  are  part  of  a 


, 


34 


sentence  to  words  which  are  not  part  of  a  sentence* 

The  scores  for  stories  three  and  eight  are  shown  in  table  I, 

Table  I 


category  story  3  story  8 

1  9  5 

2  4  2 

3  0  0 

4  0  4 

5  (2  1  ) 

6  (5  0  ) 

7  12 

8  2  3 

9  6  7 

10  2  6 

11  3  6 

12  4  5 

13  8  9 


Ten  of  the  categories  suggest  that  story  eight  is  less  dream¬ 
like  than  three*  Two  categories  {five  and  six)  suggest  that  eight 
is  more  dreamlike.  And  one  (category  three)  is  equivocal. 

These  results  do  not  prove  that  eight  is  more  imitation-like 
than  three.  The  number  of  items  in  each  is  too  small  to  establish 
significance.  And  as  far  as  we  know,  no  one  has  developed  a  precise 
method  for  determining  degrees  of  assimilation  in  thought  on  the 
basis  of  speech  alone.  In  fact,  assimilation  and  accommodation 
can  only  be  identified  by  their  symbolic  products  alone  in  the  ex¬ 
treme  cases  of  dreams  and  imitation.  The  subject  telling  a  TAT 
story  fluctuates  from  moment  to  moment  between  these  poles  of  thought 
and  the  symbolic  product  is  an  intermediate  form. 

But  our  interest  is  only  in  the  exploration  of  a  phenomenon, 
not  in  proof.  Vie  are  faced  with  two  stories  which  we  would  like  to 
place  on  a  continuum  defined  by  formal  properties.  One  way  to  de¬ 
cide  their  relative  positions  is  by  comparison  with  the  defined 
position  of  dreams.  Such  a  comparison  suggests  that,  on  the  whole. 


35 


story  three  is  relatively  more  dreamlike  than  story  eight.  Another 
way  is  to  examine  the  stories  one  at  a  time,  following  more  care¬ 
fully  the  progress  of  the  subject’s  organization  of  ideas  as  they 
are  developed  in  speech,  while  paying  closer  attention  to  his  ex¬ 
perience  of  his  own  productions.  That  is,  we  may  attempt  a  more 
direct  inspection  of  his  thought.  Let  us  apply  this  method,  begin¬ 
ning  with  story  three. 

The  first  sentence  of  three  is  a  compound  sentence,  relating 
a  past  situation  to  a  present  action.  It  is  unclear  whether  the 
"stepping  into  the  phone  booth"  is  a  consequence  of  the  disagree¬ 
ment,  or  of  the  disagreement  now  being  over.  We  expect  further  de¬ 
velopment  of  this  relationship  in  the  story,  but  the  subject  for 
the  moment  seems  primarily  concerned  with  the  act  of  stepping  into 
the  phone  booth.  He  says  "it’s  a  very  decisive  sort  of  a  thing," 
reflecting  a  certainty  about  the  act  which  we  would  expect  from  the 
declarative  and  grammatical  construction  of  the  first  sentence.  He 
seems  preoccupied  with  a  general  quality  of  the  action  and  of  the 
relationship  with  the  other  guy,  both  of  which  are  known  to  him, 
and  he  has  difficulty  building  up  context.  Not  only  is  there  a 
difficulty  with  context,  but  "I’m  not  sure.  I  can’t  tell  what  it’s 
about"  suggests  that  the  subject  experiences  the  figures  In  the 
picture  as  having  a  life  of  their  own,  with  the  facts  being  una¬ 
vailable  to  him.  He  will  have  to  make  something  up. 

The  subject  tries  by  saying  "he’s  taking  some  action  that--," 
but  the  "some"  already  indicates  uncertainty  and  the  attempt  is 
aborted.  It  is  followed  by  recourse  to  the  obvious,  "he’s  calling 
someone,"  using  clues  from  the  picture  and  placing  the  act  in  the 
present  tense,  although  even  at  the  end  of  the  story  "he"  has  not 


36 


yet  called.  There  is  the  tentative  suggestion  that  he  is  calling 
"perhaps  a  girl  they  both  know,"  followed  by  another  reference  to 
the  as  yet  unelaborated  context,  "after  disagreeing, "  of  the  call. 

The  phrase  beginning  "almost  as  a  proof  of—"  is  syntactically 
ambiguous.  Was  "his"  disagreeing  with  the  "other  guy"  a  proof  of 
his  superiority?  Or  is  his  calling  someone  a  proof  of  his  independ¬ 
ence?  Are  they  fighting  over  the  girl  at  the  other  end  of  the  line? 
None  of  these  ideas  are  clear,  despite  the  attempt  to  relate  the 
actions  of  "the  one"  to  his  intentions,  and  to  the  disagreement. 

The  attempt  to  make  use  of  a  secondary  process,  conceptual  word 
"proof"  has  an  ambiguous  result.  The  context,  sequence,  and  con¬ 
tent  of  the  disagreement  and  the  act  of  stepping  into  the  phone 
booth  are  subordinated  to  the  meaning  the  act  has  for  "the  guy." 

The  other  guy,  we  learn,  is  very  upset,  a  fact  "obvious"  to 
the  subject  by  looking  at  the  picture.  But  while  this  meaning  has 
an  unquestioned  reality,  the  attempt  to  develop  it  further,  "and 
he!s  uh,"  fails,  since  there  is  nothing  in  the  picture  to  offer  a 
way.  The  subject  appears  bound  by  the  notion  of  conflict  and  by 
the  image  of  the  card  itself.  An  attempt  to  move  temporally  beyond 
the  immediate  picture  and  its  meaning,  "as  soon  as  the  guy  gets  in¬ 
to  the  phone  booth,"  is  again  interrupted,  and  the  subject  finds 
himself  back  in  the  present,  elaborating  quality  and  meaning: 

"they3 re  right  now  sort  of  glaring  at  each  other,  in  a  way  of  say¬ 
ing  goodbye."  The  resultant  sentence  looks  slightly  ungrammatical. 

The  outcome  statement  of  part  I  is  a  well-formed  reference  to 
future  action.  It  is  basically  a  summary  statement,  however,  add¬ 
ing  a  small  piece  of  off-hand  new  information,  "the  other3 s  going 
to  leave,"  and  includes  a  last  effort  to  clarify  what  just  occurred 


37 


by  strengthening  its  meaning:  "this  final  confrontation." 

The  first  part  of  three ,  then,  is  repetitive  and  makes  use 
largely  of  simple  sentences,  the  longer  ones  being  syntactically 
ambiguous  or  slightly  ill-formed*  It  is  motionless  in  time,  and 
context  remains  undeveloped  though  not  distorted.  Very  little  in¬ 
formation  not  found  in  the  picture  is  added,  except  the  meaning  of 
the  scene,  which  the  subject  sees  as  embodied  in  the  picture.  The 
act  of  stepping  into  the  phone  booth  is  proof  of  a  point  of  view, 
glaring  is  an  expression  of  saying  goodbye,  and  a  look  is  a  sign  of 
being  upset.  There  is  a  sense  of  distance  from  detail  and  certain¬ 
ty  about  the  few  known  items,  particularly  the  importance  of  the 
action  to  both  figures.  This  sense  of  reality  binds  the  subject  to 
the  few  essential  meanings  the  picture  has  for  him.  Thus,  while 
context  remains  minimal,  there  is  a  complexity  of  meaning— superi- 
ty,  independence ,  disagreement,  saying  goodbye— which  define  a  con¬ 
flict.  This  sense  of  reality  and  conflict,  in  an  otherwise  barren 
story,  steers  the  subject  away  from  the  dreamlike  assimilation  of 
story  two. 

My  question  to  the  subject  reflects  the  fact  that  at  the  time 
I  thought  it  important  to  have  a  fuller  knowledge  of  the  subject. 
This  wasn’t  helped,  I  think,  by  the  administration  of  the  test  in 
the  room  where  medical  students  are  assigned  for  learning  psycho¬ 
therapy.  With  so  much  left  out  of  this  story,  I  responded  as  a 
puzzled  student  with  an  imprecise  research  interest  might:  I  asked 
a  question  about  feelings. 

The  subject  introduced  the  ideas  of  regret,  friendship,  and  an 
outside  sacrificial  force.  The  speech  is  correspondingly  halting: 
"and  uh  he’s  and  he  doesn’t  he’s  sort  of." 


Previously  he  focused 


t 


38 


on  the  meaning  of  the  one’s  actions  in  relation  to  the  other.  Now, 
he  is  attempting  to  get  close  enough  to  the  story  to  identify  the 
one’s  feelings,  while  remaining  far  enough  to  observe  and  express 
them.  This  change  of  stance  is  difficult  and  requires  a  change  in 
ideas,  expressed  in  a  stammering  way. 

The  speech  of  both  parts  I  and  II  of  this  story  is  different 
from  that  of  story  eight.  Let  us  examine  story  eight  and  pay  par¬ 
ticular  attention  to  the  use  of  the  black  figure.  As  we  do  so  we 
should  keep  in  mind  that  a  reliance  upon  social  reality  is  more 
indicative  of  imitative  thought  than  is  any  set  of  indices  one  might 
devise . 

The  first  sentence  of  eight  is  ungrammatical  as  it  stands,  and 
"some, some  such  place"  suggests  a  conditional  relationship  of  the 
subject  to  his  story.  However,  the  "movie  theater"  is  more  concrete 
than  most  elements  of  story  three,  and  the  subject  now  seems  to  be 
distant  from  a  situation  of  known  details. 

"A  white  guy  has  come  out,  obviously  with  money.  He’s  got 
rather  fancy  clothes  on."  The  reference  to  white  guy  and  black  guy 
is  consistent  throughout  the  story.  The  subject  is  conscious  of 
forms,  images,  and  social  groups.  The  past  tense  is  used  in  this 
first  sentence,  and  correct  and  consistent  verb  tenses  are  used 
throughout.  The  "obviously"  is  explained  by  reference  to  the  pic- 
ture,  unlike  the  "obviously"  in  story  three,  which  we  only  guessed 
was  derived  from  the  card.  There  is  both  an  awareness  of  the  details 
of  the  picture  and  a  self-conscious  sense  of  logic. 

"The  black  guy. c. and  so  he  comes  up"  is  a  rather  long,  slight¬ 
ly  ungrammatical  construction.  The  subject  is  attending  to  images 
and  social  groups.  He  is  also  self-conscious  about  his  speech  and. 


39 


attuned  to  temporal  context:  "comes  up  uh,  has  come  up."  The 
grammar  could  have  been  improved  by  replacing  "knowing"  in  each 
case  with  "he  knows,"  Also,  if  the  "knowing"  phrases  were  intended 
to  explain  some  further  action  or  thought  of  the  black  guy,  the 
repetition  of  "he  comes  up"  in  the  tense  rejected  at  the  beginning 
of  the  sentence  suggests  this  is  difficult. 

"To  spare  change  him"  is  a  noun  phrase  used  as  a  verb  phrase. 
Two  things  should  be  noted.  First,  its  use  and  the  assumption 
that  I  will  understand  depends  upon  a  rather  conventional  sense  of 
social  reality,  beyond  the  more  dictionary  meaning  of  words.  The 
phrase  is  slang  for  panhandling.  Secondly,  although  it  ostensibly 
refers  to  the  intention  of  the  black,  the  phrase  derives  as  much 
frcm  the  sought  after  possession  of  the  white.  The  black’s  inten¬ 
tions  are  seen  largely  from  the  white  guy’s  point  of  view  in  this 
story. 

In  the  next  phrases,  it  appears  that  the  black  guy’s  thoughts 
are  seen  from  this  point  of  view  as  well.  Ostensibly  "knowing  he 
can  play  on  the  other  guy’s  white  guilt,  sort  of  that  kind  of  feel¬ 
ing,,,"  is  what  the  black  is  thinking.  However,  the  sentence 
actually  tells  us  more  about  a  stereotype  of  the  white  guy.  We 
learn  of  his  "white  guilt,"  which  socializes  him  more  than  if  the 
subject  had  said  "the  white  guy’s  guilt,"  The  phrase  "increasing¬ 
ly  hard"  reflects  an  almost  historical  consciousness . 

Such  is  the  basis  on  which  the  black  guy  knows  that  he  can 
play  on  the  guilt.  There  is  no  consideration  of  what  might  be 
required  of  the  black  himself  in  order  to  approach  the  white.  This 
is  a  stereotype  not  only  of  the  black  but  also  of  the  nature  of  the 
interaction.  In  addition,  "feeling"  is  a  manifestly  inappropriate 


.  ■  * 


, 


40 


word  to  describe  the  black’s  experience,  which  is  in  the  realm  of 
"knowing,"  and  is  more  appropriate  to  "white  guilt,"  The  advantage 
cited,  furthermore,  is  in  terms  of  the  white’s  difficult  position, 
but  it  is  intended  as  information  about  the  black  guy’s  thoughts. 
Indeed,  "a  poor  looking  black"  can  only  refer  to  the  perception  of 
the  white.  This  black  guy  apparently  has  no  thoughts  of  his  own. 

As  far  as  we  know,  he  doesn  *t  need  the  money,  he  has  no  feelings, 
he  has  no  life  of  his  own,  and  no  uncertainties  about  his  act.  He 
is  just  there,  knowing  exact!}/-  what  the  white  guy  thinks. 

Next  we  hear  that  "the  white  is  feeling  very  threatened,"  not 
that  he  feels  guilty  or  generous.  The  meaning  of  the  approach  for 
the  white  as  a  person  is  different  from  the  meaning  for  him  as  a 
member  of  a  group.  The  subject  knows  what  the  black  is  up  to  since 
the  story  is  based  upon  what  he  considers  to  be  a  socially  repeated 
phenomenon.  However,  as  soon  as  one  gets  to  the  feelings  involved, 
it  is  the  white  guy  who  misperceives  the  black  and  interprets  the 
approach  as  a  threat:  "he  might  do  him  some  harm,"  The  reason 
for  this,  we  learn,  is  that  the  white  guy  reads  about  crime  all  the 
time  and  "that  sort  of  thing,"  Although  he’s  "enlightened"  in  his 
racial  views,  the  implication  Is  that  blacks  commit  crime  in  the 
streets.  His  reaction  is  determined  on  the  basis  of  newspaper  re¬ 
ports  and  blackness,  despite  the  unlikelihood  {I  think)  of  getting 
ripped  off  coming  out  of  a  movie  theater.  The  smile  near  the  be¬ 
ginning  of  the  sentence  is  ambiguous.  It  could  be  interpreted 
either  as  a  knowing  comment  on  the  unlikelihood  of  whites  having 
" enlightened"  racial  views,  the  way  one  might  react  to  a  statement 
"I’m  not  prejudiced,  but . . .  "  or  as  an  apology  for  making  such  a 


statement 


. 

, 


41 


The  contrast  is  striking  between  the  whitens  fear  of  harm, 
based  on  newspaper  reports,  and  the  black’s  actions,  based  on  know¬ 
ledge  of  the  white.  The  white  guy  sees  himself  as  a  person  with 
feelings  and  responds  to  an  imaged  act  with  socialized  meaning.  The 
black  sees  himself  through  white  eyes,  as  an  imaged  actor  with 
socialized  meanings ,  and  acts  on  this  basis. 

Sentence  six  is  complex  and  well-f ormed.  The  ’’although" 
suggests  knowledge  of  the  relationship  between  ideas  separated  in 
the  sentence.  We  learn  a  considerable  amount  about  the  life  con¬ 
text  of  the  white  guy.  There  is  a  cause  and  effect  construction, 
"and  therefore,"  and  a  relationship  expressed  between  current  feel¬ 
ings  and  past  activities.  There  is  a  recognition  of  conflicting 
thoughts,  enlightenment  versus  knowledge  of  crime,  and  conditional 
and  optative  constructions  are  part  of  the  story. 

The  next  sentence  is  equally  well-formed  and  contextual.  How¬ 
ever,  the  black  guy  now  knows  that  the  white  is  feeling  threatened 
rather  than  guilty  or  generous.  This  is  a  remarkable  man  with 
mercurial  knowledge  of  the  white’s  feelings  on  which  he  "plays," 
no  matter  what  they  are,  in  the  hope  of  getting  spare  change.  The 
white  guy  knows  nothing  about  the  black,  while  the  black’s  knowledge 
dovetails  the  white’s  feelings  exactly.  The  black  is  consistent  as 
a  form,  engaged  in  an  act,  and  his  meaning  is  a  reflection  of  the 
meaning  he  has  for  the  white.  The  subject  is  slightly  aware  of 
this,  saying  "pretty  much  knows,"  but  he  is  not  aware  enough  to 
construct  a  different  story. 

The  last  few  lines  deal  primarily  with  the  white’s  departure. 
The  speech  is  slightly  more  halting  and  slightly  less  complex  than 
the  preceding  sentences.  The  subject  is  mildly  uncertain  about  one 


42 


element  not  pictured,  "a  girl,11  of  whom  the  subject  says  "I  think" 
rather  than  "I'll  say,"  again  reflecting,  as  in  the  first  sentence, 
a  distance  from  a  story  with  known  details.  Sentence  ten,  "it  sort 
of  doubles  his  fear  but  it  also  doubles  his  courage"  was  spoken 
rapidly  without  fearful  affect.  It  is  a  conventional  expression, 
corresponding  in  speech  to  the  subject's  use  of  social  context— the 
girl  and  the  car— in  thought.  What  is  social  now  has  the  meaning 
of  security.  What  is  external  supports  at  every  turn  the  feelings 
and  actions  of  the  white  guy. 

Like  the  black  woman  in  need,  the  black  man  participates  in  a 
defensive  transformation.  But  the  formal  aspect  of  this  transforma¬ 
tion  is  of  a  very  different  type.  It  is  an  imitation-like  shift  to 
a  contextually  conventional  and  linguistically  well-organized  con¬ 
struction.  The  black  figure  is  largely  a  personless  social  form. 

By  contrast,  the  transf ormation  from  story  nine  to  story  two  is  a 
dreamlike  transf ormation:  The  black  figure  is  a  meaningful  but 
formless  human  being.  It  appears  that  a  racial  difference  may  assist 

to  dominance  either  of  two  extremes:  dreamlike  imagination  or 
im i t  at i on- 1 i k e  c  on v en t i onal i t  y , 

This  impression  is  supported  by  an  exmaination  of  the  remain¬ 
ing  five  pairs  of  stories.  Each  story  was  scored  separately  in 
each  of  the  thirteen  categories  we  are  using.  Then  the  categories 
were  taken  one  at  a  time  and  the  stories  were  scored  in  succession. 
Each  category  required  one  trip  through  the  entire  protocol.  The 


43 


results  from  the  two  methods  were  compared,  and  a  final  reading  of 
the  protocol  reconciled  the  discrepancies.  Table  II  shows  the  re¬ 
sults  of  the  scoring.  The  scores  in  each  column  have  been  correct¬ 
ed  for  the  difference  in  the  number  of  ivords  between  the  members  of 
each  pair  of  stories.  If  x=the  number  of  words  in  the  shorter  story 
and  y=the  number  of  words  in  its  corresponding  longer  story,  the 
raw  scores  for  the  shorter  story  have  been  multiplied  by  the  factor 
y/x,  except,  of  course,  in  category  thirteen. 

Table  II 


Cat , 

2B 

9W 

8B 

3W 

10B 

1W 

12B 

4W 

53 

11W 

13B 

6W  7B 

14W 

1 

18 

5 

5 

9*9 

3.2 

13 

8 

4.4 

21 

10.2 

5 

14.6  (2,1 

2) 

2 

10 

2 

2 

4.4 

2.1 

5 

4 

3*3 

10 

7.3 

0 

6.2  4.2 

5 

3 

3 

0 

0 

0 

0 

0 

0 

0 

1 

0 

0 

2,1  0 

1 

4 

1 

6 

4 

0 

6 , 4 

3 

1 

2,3 

0 

8.7 

4 

2,1  5.3 

5 

5 

0 

1 

(1 

2 .  3 )  ( 2 , 1 

3) 

0 

1,1 

0 

4,4 

(0 

1)  (o 

1) 

6 

1 

6 

(0 

5.7) 

0 

0 

0 

0 

0 

1 .5 

1 

0  ( 1. 1 

2) 

7 

0 

4 

2 

1.1 

4.2 

0 

0 

1.1 

0 

1.5 

1 

0  (0 

3) 

8 

2 

4 

3 

2,2 

4,2 

2 

0 

1.1 

4 

5 » 8 

5 

2.1  5.3 

4 

9 

5 

5 

7 

6,  8 

(7.4 

9) 

(4 

3.3) 

2 

13.1 

8 

4.2  10,6 

8 

10 

2 

11 

6 

2.3 

10,6 

3 

1 

4.4 

(6 

l.5)(3 

3.1)  6.4 

6 

11 

1 

9 

6 

3.3 

6 .4 

4 

1 

2,3 

4 

5.8 

(1 

6.2)  5.3 

4 

12 

0 

3 

5 

4.4 

(3.2 

4) 

(3 

0) 

0 

4,4 

4 

3.1  4.2 

4 

13 

3 

6. 5 

9 

8 

20 

5.5 

2.5 

9 

(10 

8) 

14 

9  9 

4 

D 

I 

I 

D 

I 

D 

D 

I 

D 

I 

I 

D  I 

D 

Since  we  are  dealing  with  relative  differences,  every  dream¬ 
like  transf ormation  with  black  forms  implies  an  imitation-like 
transf ormation  with  white  forms.  Similarly,  every  imitation-like 
transf ormation  with  black  forms  implies  a  dreamlike  transf ormation 
with  white  forms.  Thus,  for  each  pair  of  stories  we  label  one  "I" 
for  imitation-like  and  one  "D"  for  dreamlike.  The  choice  is  deter¬ 
mined  by  the  frequency  of  shifts  one  direction  or  the  other 
throughout  the  thirteen  categories.  Scores  which  contradict  the 
overall  trend  for  any  pair  are  bracketed  in  table  II.  In  no  pair 
does  the  number  of  contradictory  shifts  exceed  four.  The  pair  two 


44 


and  nine  is  the  most  clear-cut,  with  only  one  equivocal  shift. 

The  pair  seven  and  fourteen  presents  the  least  decree  of  transforma¬ 
tion,  with  four  contradictory  categories  and  no  equivocal  ones. 

We  are  interested  in  whether  black  forms  assist  transf ormat ions 
toward  opposite  extremes  of  a  continuum.  If  we  were  merely  to 
compare  the  most  extreme  stories  of  both  poles,  with  and  without 
black  forms,  we  could  not  determine  whether  the  effects  observed 
were  a  consequence  of  race  or  not,  since  we  would  be  comparing 
stories  told  to  figures  of  different  age  and  sex.  Neither  could 
we  simply  average  the  scores  from  the  B  cards  and  compare  these  with 
the  averages  for  the  W  cards.  Extremes  would  cancel  each  other  out 
by  this  method.  We  may,  however,  compare  the  group  of  dreamlike 
transf ormations  with  B  cards  and  the  group  of  dreamlike  transforma¬ 
tions  with  W  cards  by  averaging  the  scores  for  each  category  with¬ 
in  the  group.  Table  III  shows  these  results  and  the  similar  ones 
for  imitation-like  stories. 


m 

-L 

able 

III 

cat , 

D 

I 

B 

W 

B 

W 

1 

15.7 

9.8 

3.8 

6,5 

2 

6 

5.2 

2,1 

4.1 

3 

1.3 

,8 

0 

0 

4 

.7 

2,5 

(4,9 

5.6) 

5 

0 

1.8 

(  .8 

2.2) 

6 

.3 

1.9 

(  .5 

2.5) 

7 

0 

1,0 

(1.8 

2,2) 

8 

2 

2 , 6 

4.4 

3.6 

9 

3.7 

7 

8.2 

7.1 

10 

3 

3.7 

6.5 

5 . 6 

11 

2 

4,4 

(4,7 

5.7) 

12 

1 

3.9 

4.1 

2.7 

13 

5.1 

6.6 

1  3 

7,6 

For  every  category  within  the  D  group  the  B  stories  are  more 
dreamlike  than  the  W  stories.  And  for  the  majority  of  categories  with¬ 
in  the  I  group,  the  B  stories  are  more  imitation-like  than  the  W  stories. 


4-5 


We  see,  however,  that  there  are  five  categories  in  the  I 
columns  of  table  III  which  contradict  the  overall  trend,,  Part  of 
the  difficulty  here  is  in  the  selection  of  the  indices  of  imitation, 
A  construction  which  is  "less  dreamlike"  is  not  necessarily  "more 
imitation-like,"  since  there  is  the  third  possibility  that  a  con¬ 
struction  may  reflect  "conceptual  thought"  as  defined  in  the  next 
section.  And  as  we  stated  on  page  38,  a  reliance  upon  social 
reality  is  the  surest  indicator  of  imitation. 

Nevertheless,  our  main  interest  is  in  the  relative  potential  of 
black  and  white  forms  to  assist  constructional  divergence.  We  may 
compare,  then,  the  changes  from  D  to  I  within  the  set  of  B  stories, 
to  the  corresponding  changes  within  the  set  of  V/  stories,  as  shown 
in  table  IV, 


J 

cat 

1 

D 

15.7 

CQ 

00 

M  » 

CO 

Table  IV 

£>B 

11.9  9.8 

A  W 

I 

6 . 5 

W 

3.3 

A  B 

>  A 

W 

2 

6 

2,1 

3.9 

5,2 

4.1 

lol 

A  B 

>  A 

W 

3 

1.3 

0 

1.3 

,8 

0 

08 

A  B 

>  A 

W 

4 

,7 

4.9 

4,2 

2.5 

5 . 6 

3,1 

A  B 

>  A 

w 

5 

0 

.8 

,8 

1,8 

2.2 

.4 

A  B 

>  A 

w 

6 

. 3 

.5 

,  2 

1.9 

2.5 

06 

(  A  B 

4  A 

w) 

7 

0 

1.8 

1,8 

1.0 

2.2 

1.2 

A  B 

>A 

w 

8 

2 

4,4 

2.4 

2 . 6 

3,6 

1,0 

A  B 

>  A 

w 

9 

3.7 

8,2 

4.5 

7 

7.1 

.1 

AB 

>  A 

w 

10 

3 

6,5 

3*5 

3»7 

5 » 6 

1.9 

A  B 

>A 

w 

11 

2 

4,7 

2.7 

4.4 

5.7 

1.3 

A  B 

>  A 

w 

12 

1 

4,1 

3.1 

(3*9 

2.7) 

-1,2 

AB 

>  A 

w 

13 

5,1 

13 

7.9 

6,6 

7.6 

1 

6  B 

>  A 

w 

There 

are  no 

categories 

within  the 

B  set 

which 

contradict 

the 

general  trend  from  D  to  I,  There  is  one  such  contradict ion  within 
the  more  balanced  W  set.  More  importantly,  in  every  category  but  one 
the  degree  of  change  in  the  B  set  exceeds  the  degree  of  change  in  the 
W  set.  It  appears  that  black  forms  assist  the  loss  of  equilibrium 
in  thought,  as  we  will  now  discuss. 


46 


III 

The  Creation  of  Racial  Opposites 

In  the  last  section  we  observed  that  fantasy  life  is  transform¬ 
ed  under  the  impact  of  race,  A  visual  racial  property  of  a  photo¬ 
graphic  image  enables  thought  to  reach  divergent  extremes.  The 
transf ormations  are  defensive  in  content  and  beyond  self-awareness. 
To  understand  such  facts  we  need  an  account  of  symbolic  process 
which  conceptualizes  an  interplay  of  external  forms  with  patterned 
ideas  and  a  relationship  between  alternative  modes  of  construction.. 
Jean  Piaget,  in  his  study  of  cognitive  growth,  has  developed  such 
an  account. 

Central  to  Piaget’s  theory  is  the  notion  that  mental  activity 
in  interaction  with  the  world  of  objects  is  both  structured  and 
structuring.  From  the  moment  of  birth,  individual  patterns  of 
activity— called  "schema" --may  be  evoked,  reinforced,  and  elaborated 
by  the  forms  of  the  external  world.  That  is,  a  child  may  actively 
"accommodate"  schema  to  these  forms.  Alternatively  a  child  may  modi¬ 
fy,  or  structure,  external  forms  according  to  his  own  activity. 

That  is,  a  child  may  "assimilate"  external  forms  to  a  schema.  Fi¬ 
nally,  a  child  may  both  modify  and  adjust  to  external  forms  simul¬ 
taneously.  In  this  case  assimilation  and  accommodation— -the  two 
functional  techniques  for  the  transf ormat ion  of  structures— are 
said  to  be  in  "equilibrium,"  This  is  the  state  characteristic  of 
adaptive  intelligence ,  An  example  from  Piaget’s  Play,  Dreams,  and 
Imitation  in  Childhood  illustrates  these  processes  at  the  level  of 


sensory-motor  activity. 


, 


47 


As  we  have  just  reminded  our  readers,  intelligence 
tends  towards  permanent  equilibrium  between  assimilation  and 
accommodation.  For  instance,  in  order  to  draw  an  objective 
towards  him  by  means  of  a  stick,  the  child  must  assimilate 
both  stick  and  objective  to  the  schema  of  prehension  and  that 
of  movement  through  contact,  and  he  must  also  accommodate 
these  schemas  to  the  objects,  their  length,  distance,  etc., 
in  accordance  with  the  causal  order  hand-stick-objective... 
Imitation  (the  primacy  of  accommodation  over  assimilation) 
will  reproduce  the  motion  made  by  the  stick  in  reaching  the 
objective,  the  movement  of  the  hand  thus  being  determined 
by  those  of  the  stick  and  the  objective  (which  is  by  defini¬ 
tion  accommodation) ,  without  the  hand  actually  affecting  the 
objects  (which  would  be  assimilation).  There  is,  however, 
a  third  possibility,  that  of  assimilation  per  se .  Let  us 
assume,  for  instance,  that  the  stick  does  not  reach  its  ob¬ 
jective  and  that  the  child  consoles  himself  by  hitting  some¬ 
thing  else,  or  that  he  suddenly  becomes  interested  in  moving 
the  stick  for  its  own  sake,  or  that  when  he  has  no  stick  he 
takes  a  piece  of  paper  and  applies  the  schema  of  the  stick 
to  it  for  fun... In  such  cases  there  is  a  kind  of  free  assimi¬ 
lation,  without  accommodation  to... the  significance  of  ob¬ 
jects.  1 

We  should  note  in  this  passage  that  in  addition  to  the  assim¬ 
ilation  of  external  objects  to  the  schemas  of  prehension  and  move¬ 
ment,  these  two  schemas  are  themselves  coordinated  with  one  another. 
That  is,  they  are  mutually  assimilated.  Similarly,  if  the  child’s 
activity  is  to  be  intelligent  and  tend  toward  equilibrium,  the  accom¬ 
modations  to  the  stick  and  objective  must  be  coordinated  by  an 
assimilatory  process.  As  intelligent  as  such  activity  is,  however, 
it  does  not  deserve  to  be  called  11  representative  activity,"  since 
it  operates  only  upon  objects  within  immediate  perception.  Never¬ 
theless,  the  techniques  illustrated  are  responsible  for  the  gradual 
unfolding  of  more  complex  cognitive  processes  out  of  the  matrix  of 
sensory-motor  activity.  Cognitive  growth,  from  this  point  of  viex-tf, 


1 

•Jean  Piaget,  Play.  Dreams,  and '  Imitation  in  Childhood,  translated 
by  G.  Gattegno  and  F.M.  Hodgson,  W.W.  Norton  and  Company,  Inc., 

New  York,  1962,  Brackets(  )  are  mine,  D.B. 


48 


is  conceived  as  the  continuous  elaboration  of  prior  structures,  De 
velopment  is  not  merely  the  successive  emergence  of  de  novo  abili¬ 
ties,  And  it  is  not  the  consequence  of  neurological  development 
alone  or  of  mysterious  transfers  from  social  life  alone. 

With  these  few  simple  ideas  in  hand,  Piaget  explores  an  enor¬ 
mously  complex  and  subtle  cognitive  landscape.  For  the  purposes 
of  this  paper  it  is  only  necessary  to  discuss  two  aspects  of  cogni¬ 
tive  development:  the  emergence  of  representation  and  the  charac¬ 
teristics  of  the  child1 s  egocentric  thought. 

Representative  activity  begins  when  the  child  becomes  able  to 
evoke  an  absent  object  or  model.  At  a  particular  stage,  a  child  be 
comes  capable  of  "deferred  imitation,"  based  on  " interiorized 
accommodation."  That  is,  he  becomes  able  to  imitate  an  external 
model,  say  a  parent  clapping  his  hands,  for  the  first  time  after 
the  model  has  disappeared  from  sight.  Coordination  of  the  schemas 
for  extension  of  the  fingers  and  arm  movement,  and  accommodation 
of  these  to  the  hand  clapping  has  occurred  internally,  in  the  form 
of  suggestions  of  action,  rather  than  actual  action.  This  accom¬ 
modated  schema  is  a  virtual  combination  of  movements.  It  is  a 
"draft,"  "summary,"  or  "negative"  of  the  model.  It  is  also  a 
potential  image,  since  the  model  can  now  be  evoked,  either  as  image 
or  in  later  action,  by  utilizing  the  accommodated  schema  in  some 
way. 

Such  accommodated  schemas  constitute  a  system  of  "signifiers" 
for  the  child®s  thought.  By  virtue  of  their  dif f erentiation  from 
and  persistence  beyond  actual  performance,  they  acquire  a  certain 
mobility.  They  are  the  basis  of  the  image  and  deferred  imitation. 
Furthermore,  they  may  be  combined  and  coordinated  with  other  past 


49 


and  present  signifiers,  and  therby  acquire  symbolic  meaning.  The 
schemas  of  coordination-really ,  assimilation-provide  the  signifiers 
with  meaning,  and  are  defined  as  the  "signified."  By  the  differen¬ 
tiation  of  signifier  from  signified,  the  child’s  world  expands  in 
space  and  time.  This  is  the  symbolic  function. 

Consider  a  child  attempting  to  dance.  A  record  is  playing  as 
the  child  watches  an  older  sister  doing  a  step,  which  he  attempts 
to  imitate  by  jumping  up  and  down.  A  number  of  assimilations  and 
accommodations  are  in  progress.  The  child  is  accommodating  audi¬ 
tory  schemas  to  the  sound,  and  visual  and  motor  schemas  to  the  move¬ 
ments  of  the  model.  These  accommodated  schemas  are  signifiers. 

They  must  be  mutually  assimilated,  however,  to  be  united  in  meaning, 
that  is,  to  form  the  child’s  sensory-motor  notion  of  the  dance. 

This  assimilatory  activity  constitutes  the  signified. 

Now,  if  the  older  sister  stops  dancing  and  the  child  stops  also, 
and  we  assume  that  the  child  is  unable  to  continue  in  the  absence 
of  the  model,  we  cannot  attribute  representative  activity  to  the 
child.  The  child  appears  able  to  accommodate  to  the  model  and  assim¬ 
ilate  the  model  to  his  schemas  of  movement  only  when  the  model  is 
actually  present.  In  other  words,  the  signif iers-~the  schemas  of 
accommodation  to  the  sound  and  sight— and  the  signif ied— the  schemas 
of  assimilation  of  these  to  the  child’s  activity  are  not  sufficiently 
differentiated.  There  is  no  permanent  image  of  the  object  apart 
from  the  perception  of  the  model. 

Now,  suppose  that  the  child  was  only  watching  the  older  sister 
dance,  and  when  she  sits  down,  the  child  begins  to  jump  up  and  down 
to  the  music.  Such  an  action  exhibits  representative  activity. 

This  activity  consists  of  a  twofold  system  of  accommodations  and 


50 


assimilations,  past  and  present,  in  a  relationship  with  one  another . 
The  first  system  was  described  above  and  is  now  assumed  to  have  pro¬ 
ceeded  interiorly,  and  persist  in  the  form  of  an  integrated  system 
of  potential  images.  The  second  system  consists  of  the  child's 
assimilation  and  accommodation  of  the  present  ob jects-~the  music 
and  the  child's  body-- to  certain  schemas  of  movement. 

These  two  systems  are  related  to  one  another  in  thought.  The 
present  objects  (the  music  and  the  child's  body)  are  related  to  the 
past  objects  (the  sister  and  the  music)  by  means  of  assimilation  of 
the  music  and  body  to  the  previously  accommodated  schema.  This 
assimilation  of  present  to  past  endows  the  present  objects  with 
meaning  as  dance.  The  signified  schema  is  the  assimilatory  inter¬ 
mediary  between  the  two  sets  of  signifiers.  It  should  be  clear  that 
this  arrangement  would  be  impossible  if  the  system  of  signifiers 
and  signified  were  not  somewhat  mobile  and  independent  of  each  other. 
Assimilation  of  the  present  objects  to  the  schema  of  dance,  and 
accommodation  of  this  schema  to  the  present  objects  could  not  happen 
if  accommodation  to  the  model  were  simultaneously  required,  since 
the  model  is  now  absent. 

When  the  representative  activity  of  the  prelogical  child  (call¬ 
ed  egocentric  representation)  is  characterized  by  the  primacy  of 
assimilation  over  accommodation,  the  activity  is  manifest  as  symbolic 
play;  or,  in  more  extreme  assimilatory  activity,  as  dreams.  If  our 
dancing  child  now  sits  at  the  dinner  table  and  bounces  a  glass  up 
and  down,  saying  "sister-dance,"  he  subordinates  his  interest  in  the 
objective  properties  of  the  glass  to  his  interest  in  the  absent 
model.  He  is  playing.  He  is  distorting  the  qualities  of  the  glass 
and  the  table  to  evoke  the  model  of  the  sister  dancing.  In  contrast 


51 


to  the  situation  described  above,  the  model  of  the  sister  dancing 
is  not  evoked  by  an  imitative  reproduction  of  it  but  by  means  of  a 
weakly  similar  intermediate  object  to  which  the  qualities  of  the 
signified  model  are  attributed.  This  is  symbolic  play.  It  depends 
upon  the  union  of  imaged  signifiers  (accommodated  schemas)  which 
evoke  the  absent  model,  with  a  system  of  meanings  by  which  these 
are  related  to  present  objects,  also  used  as  signifiers.  These  pre¬ 
sent  signifiers  and  the  accommodated  schema  to  which  they  are  assim¬ 
ilated  together  form  the  symbol. 

The  symbolization  of  dreams,  and  of  egocentric  "preconceptual" 
thought  in  general,  involve  what  are  called  "motivated  symbols." 
Signifiers  are  a  substitute  for  what  is  signified.  Assimilation  of 
present  objects  to  the  schemas  of  past  models  depends  upon  some 
imagined  resemblance,  since  at  this  level  representation  requires 
the  assistance  of  images.  What  distinguishes  this  imaged  symboli¬ 
zation  from  conceptual  signification  is  not  the  relative  dominance 
of  assimilation  or  accommodation,  which  are  merely  two  techniques 
available  to  thought  on  all  levels.  Rather,  egocentric  and  concep¬ 
tual  thought  are  dist inguished  by  the  nature  of  the  relationship  be¬ 
tween  assimilating  and  accommodat ing  structures. 

Let  us  return  to  the  child9 s  schema  of  dance,  A  child  who  can 
recognize  other  members  of  his  family  as  dancing  by  virtue  of  the 
similarity  of  their  movements  to  those  of  the  older  sister,  has  form¬ 
ed  a  preconcept  of  "dance,"  The  stability  of  the  class  of  "dancing 
objects"  depends  upon  the  image  of  the  sister  dancing,  which  is  a 
"privileged  signifier"  for  the  signified  preconcept.  This  precon¬ 
ceptual  structure  accommodates  and  assimilates  those  objects  with 
rather  specific  perceptual  qualities,  perhaps  a  certain  sound,  or 


52 


a  repeating  movement.  Without  some  resemblance  to  a  particular 
aspect  of  the  model,  another  type  of  rhythmical  movement  to  music 
may  not  be  assimilated  to  the  schema  "dance,'1  The  sister  doing  a 
different  step  may  not  be  dancing. 

True  generality  is,  theref ore,  not  achieved  by  the  preconceptual 
structure.  Similarly,  true  individuality  of  the  elements  of  the 
class  "dancing  objects"  is  not  achieved,  since  a  dance  is  appreciated 
as  such  only  insofar  as  it  is  similar  to  the  privileged  signifier. 
Insofar  as  it  is  different,  It  is  not  a  dance, 

A  conceptual  structure,  on  the  other  hand,  would  accommodate 
these  different  forms  of  dance  equivalently  and  reversibly,  with  the 
assistance  of  verbal  signs.  Each  object  would  be  involved  in  dance 
only  by  virtue,  say  of  the  rhythmical  coordination  of  body  with 
music.  Comparison  of  one  type  of  dance  with  another  would  not  be 
necessary  to  identify  both  as  dances.  The  existence  of  a  general 
concept  of  dance  would  serve  to  preserve  the  uniqueness  and  integ¬ 
rity  of,  say,  the  "Penguin"  alone  or  in  comparison  with  the  "Fox 
Trot,"  It  would  also  serve  to  define  certain  activities  as  "dance" 
or  "not-dance, "  regardless  of  the  participation  of  the  subject. 

Prior  to  the  development  of  such  concepts  the  child9 s  thought 
remains  centered  on  his  own  point  of  view.  Accommodation  is  primarily 
of  new  models  which  are  both  interesting  to  the  child  and  identical 
or  analagous  to  his  own  schema  of  assimilation.  By  virtue  of  being 
centered  on  the  child 5 s  point  of  view,  thought  at  this  level  always 
involves  the  real  or  imagined  participation  of  the  child.  It  is 
image-bound.  When  a  volume  of  water  is  poured  from  one  container 
into  a  container  of  a  different  shape,  the  preconceptual  child  will 
assert  that  the  amount  of  water  has  changed.  Lacking  an  adequate 


53 


conservational  structure,  he  is  unable  to  accommodate  to  both  images 
simultaneously.  He  is  unable  to  preserve  different  points  of  view 
and  transcend  his  stance  of  the  moment.  For  this  reason  equilibrium 
is  unstable.  The  child  continually  swings  back  and  forth  between 
imitative  accommodation  and  assimilatory  play,  alternately  taking 
the  point  of  view  of  social  reality  and  personal  reality.  Intelli¬ 
gent  adaptation  is  a  transient  way  station  lying  between  these 
oscillations  of  the  mind.  Equilibrium  acquires  stability  only  with 
increasing  degrees  of  accommodation  to  reality,  and  therefore  only 
with  the  development  of  increasingly  mobile  assimilatory  structures. 

In  other  words,  stable  equilibrium  becomes  possible  as  thought  be¬ 
comes  socialized. 

From  this  notion  of  symbolic  process,  we  can  see  that  the 
dreamlike  and  imitation-like  swings  of  racial  fantasy  constitute  a 
"symbolic  regression"  to  a,  more  egocentric  state  of  mind  than  is 
seen  in  the  nonracial  fantasy.  Our  subject  thinks  less  intelligent¬ 
ly  with  racially  mixed  forms  than  with  all  white  forms.  To  under¬ 
stand  why  this  can  happen,  we  must  examine  the  projective  test  situa¬ 
tion  in  terms  of  Piaget’s  theory,  and  reconsider  the  process  of  de¬ 
fensive  fantasy. 

The  projective  test  situation  demands  the  suspension  of  intelli¬ 
gence  in  favor  of  pretense.  The  subject  must  think  about  photographic 
figures  as  if  they  are  real.  Since  the  figures  are  motionless 
images  with  intrinsically  ambiguous  meanings  (different  subjects 
tell  different  stories),  accommodation  to  them  contributes  almost 
nothing  to  the  story  line.  They  are  largely  assimilated  to  previous 
signifiers  which,  in  turn,  have  been  assimilated  to  even  earlier 
ones,  and  so  on.  Nevertheless,  the  assimilation  of  the  figures  to 


54 


human  signifiers  suggests  a  minimal  accommodation  to  the  properties 
of  the  figures  on  the  card.  Our  subject  did  not  tell  stories  about 
animals,  plants,  or  rocks.  His  stories  were  built  on  past  assimila¬ 
tions  and  accommodations  of  people,  for  which  the  present  forms  are 
"motivated"  symbols  by  virtue  of  imagined  similarity  to  them.  The 
subject  can  then  regard  his  story  as  being  at  least  plausible  in 
terms  of  his  knowledge  about  people. 

The  stories  cannot,  however,  become  too  plausible,  and  the 
subject  must  preserve  a  degree  of  pretense.  On  the  one  hand,  he 
must  avoid  the  experience  of  merely  recounting  memories  of  past 
events.  On  the  other,  he  must  avoid  reducing  his  stories  to  simple 
descriptions  of  the  cards.  This  requires  the  maintenance  of  a  dual 
sense  of  difference.  Initially,  the  difference  is  between  the  fig¬ 
ures  on  the  cards  and  the  people  who  he  knows .  As  thought  proceeds, 
it  becomes  the  difference  between  the  assembled  signifiers  of  his 
story  and  both  of  the  initially  distinct  sets.  This  enables  the 
subject  to  tell  a  story  which  is  both  real  and  make-believe.  His 
extreme  distortions  become  tolerable,  and  egocentric  thought,  cen¬ 
tered  on  the  subject* s  point  of  view,  is  free  to  proceed. 

In  this  situation  the  subject  creates  stories  lying  between 
two  extremes,  corresponding  to  two  alternative  modes  of  construction 
and  reflecting  the  relative  dominance  of  assimilation  or  accommoda¬ 
tion. 

When  assimilation  is  dominant,  a  dreamlike  story  is  created. 
Bits  and  pieces  of  past  experience  are  gathered  together  with  dis¬ 
regard  for  context.  The  properties  of  the  signifiers  are  subordi¬ 
nated  to  the  meaning  (the  signified)  the  subject  has  in  mind.  In 
story  number  two,  for  instance,  what  is  important  is  the  girl's  ex- 


55 


perience  of  a  "tough  time,"  The  signifiers  for  the  "tough  time" 
run  the  gamut  from  an  impossible  test  and  a  friend5 s  problems  to 
the  death  of  her  mother. 

Despite  the  resultant  incongruities ,  belief  is  preserved  by  the 
sense  of  important  meaning  and  closeness  to  the  subject 5 s  personal 
reality.  The  subject  runs  the  risk,  however,  of  the  meaning  becoming 
identical  with  the  objects  of  the  signified  schema,  the  "privileged" 
signifiers.  In  story  two  the  vague  reference  to  "something  she’s 
thought  about  their  relationship"  is  followed  by  a  hasty  retreat. 

Such  an  idea  can  be  explicitly  stated  in  story  nine,  and  tolehated 
there  since  the  basis  of  belief  depends  less  upon  the  importance  of 
meaning  in  that  story.  What  rescues  him  in  story  two  Is  an  assim- 
ilatory  spree,  which  reestablishes  the  uniqueness  of  his  story  and 
sustains  artificiality.  The  confirmation  of  pretense  relies  upon 
the  difference  between  the  signified  objects  and  the  forms  on  the 
cards.  This  difference  is  used  by  the  subject  to  unlock  assimila¬ 
tion,  and  restore  the  difference  between  the  characters  of  his  story 
and  the  signified  objects.  The  difference  acquires  meaning.  Hence, 
in  this  type  of  story  the  cards  can  be  used  for  their  meaning  value 
only  if  the  subject  remains  anchored  in  his  latent  awareness  of 
their  formal  properties. 

At  the  other  extreme  is  the  imitation-like  story  created  under 
the  dominance  of  accomodation.  The  context  of  preexistent  signifiers 
is  preserved  and  well  organized  in  accordance  with  social  reality. 
Here  it  is  not  so  much  a  question  of  selecting  scattered  details  to 
fit  a  signified  schema,  but  of  selecting  a  schema  of  signification 
which  may  be  embodied  in  social  context.  Such  a  story  is  believable 
because  it  relies  so  heavily  on  previous  accommodations  to  social 


56 


reality. 

At  the  same  time,  such  reliance  on  accommodation  continually 
threatens  belief,  since  the  difference  between  the  TAT  forms  and 
the  characters  of  his  story  is  ever  apparent .  In  story  nine,  for 
instance,  the  subject  is  always  explaining  himself.  The  white  guy 
has  money  because  "he9s  got  rather  fancy  clothes  on."  He  fears 
harm  because  of  "crime  and  that  sort  of  thing  that  he  reads  about 
all  the  time."  The  assertion  of  "enlightened  racial  views"  requires 
an  apology.  Accommodation  continually  highlights  the  subject 9 s 
awareness  of  his  own  creative  acts,  and,  therefore,  the  personal 
source  of  his  meanings ,  His  explanations  resocialize  his  thought 
by  capitalizing  on  context  and  minimizing  the  difference  between 
the  cards  and  previously  accommodated  social  reality.  Hence,  in 
this  type  of  story  the  subject  can  use  the  pictures  for  their  value 
as  social  forms,  only  by  maintaining  a  latent  awareness  of  the 
similarity  between  the  photographic  images  and  real  people. 

When  assimilation  is  dominant,  pretense  threatens  to  be  over¬ 
whelmed  by  belief.  But  when  accommodation  reigns,  belief  threatens 
to  be  overwhelmed  by  pretense.  Both  problems  are  solved  by  the  use 
of  formal  context.  Both  solutions  make  use  of  human  similarity  and 
perceptual  difference. 

The  creation  of  one  or  the  other  types  of  stories  is  a  reflec¬ 
tion  of  the  concerns  of  the  subject.  To  understand  the  nature  of 
the  alternative,  we  must  redefine  notions  of  conflict  and-  defense 
in  terms  of  symbolic  process. 

Conflicting  ideas  can  be  defined  as  those  which  lead  to  incom¬ 
patible  ends.  Our  subject,  like  everyone  else,  can  imagine  himself 
in  relation  to  others.  He  has  had  this  ability  since  the  beginnings 


r  X  ; 

. 


5? 


of  representative  activity,  and  it  emerged  gradually  from  the 
sensory-motor  object  relations  of  infancy.  Over  the  years  of  child¬ 
hood,  structured  notions  of  gratifying  relations  to  objects  gradu¬ 
ally  unfold.  These  are  "wishes."  In  addition,  there  is  the  struc¬ 
tured  elaboration,  under  the  impact  of  interpersonal  life,  of  various 
notions  of  loss.  These  are  "fears."  Contradiction  and  difficulty 
arise  out  of  the  assimilation  of  these  effectively  important  schemas 
with  one  another. 

The  two  simplest  cases  involve  the  assimilation  of  two  conflict¬ 
ing  wishes  and  the  assimilation  of  a  wish  and  a  fear.  In  the  first 
case,  the  imagined  fulfillment  of  one  wish  creates  a  self-object 
situation  which  is  different  from  the  objective  of  the  other  wish. 
Similarly,  in  the  second  case  assimilation  of  the  object  to  either 
schema  precludes  assimilation  to  the  other.  In  both  cases,  the 
conflict  is  only  experienced  when  the  objective  properties  of  the 
object  situation  are  taken  into  account.  The  conflict,  then  reduces 
to  that  between  personal  reality  and  perceived  social  actuality, 
between  the  assimilation  of  objects  and  the  accommodations  such  ob¬ 
jects  demand.  Loss  only  becomes  a  possibility  when  social  reality 
is  taken  into  account.  When  it  is  not  important,  as  in  dreams, 
wishes  are  fulfilled. 

Psychoanalysis  has  studied  the  persistence  of  these  conflicts 
throughout  life.  It  has  discovered  that  effectively  significant 
structures  of  assimilation  are  forever  nourished  by  instinctual 
drive,  "the  body9s  demand  on  the  mind  for  work,"  yet  forever  de¬ 
prived  (at  the  preconceptual  level)  of  true  equilibrium  with  social 
forms.  Furthermore,  with  each  experience  of  conflict,  new  layers 
of  structured  signifiers  are  created,  always  built  upon  the  founda- 


58 


tions  of  previous  structures.  These  fantasies  serve  a  defensive 
function.  From  our  point  of  view,  they  resolve  or  mediate  the 
discontinuity  between  personal  and  social  reality.  Their  creation 
requires  symbolic  thought. 

As  always,  there  are  two  techniques  available  to  the  mind  for 
the  achievement  of  its  defensive  goal,  and  we  may  differentiate  be¬ 
tween  defensive  accommodation  and  defensive  assimilation.  Defensive 
accommodation  entails  the  modification  of  all  or  part  of  an  assim- 
ilatory  schema  (the  imagined  self-object-relationship)  according  to 
the  pattern  of  an  external  relationship.  By  this  technique,  a 
separation  becomes  a  distinction.  An  oversimplified  example  might 
be  the  late  oedipal  child 9 s  accommodation  to  the  social  situation 
of  his  family.  The  child  modifies  his  behavior  primarily  on  the 
model  of  one  adult  figure,  under  the  impact  of  separation  from 
another.  In  general,,  defensive  accommodation  replaces  a  difficult 
separation  by  a  tolerable  difference  as  the  person  becomes  a  partic¬ 
ipant  in  social  structure. 

This  technique  requires  the  subordination  of  distorting  person¬ 
alized  meanings  to  rigid  and  conventional  socialized  meanings.  For 
it  to  be  achieved  at  all,  however,  the  accommodated  signifiers  must 
be  both  similar  to  the  signified  objects,  and  yet  different  enough 
and  general  enough  to  mask  the  personalized  meanings  which  would 
reawaken  old  conflict.  It  must  be  possible  to  detach  meanings  based 
on  imaged  properties  from  meanings  based  on  secret  schemas  of  assim¬ 
ilation,  This  is  achieved  by  conscious  utilization  of  formal  dif¬ 
ferences,  with  an  unconscious  use  of  similarity.  It  amounts  to  a 
psychic  cleavage  of  form  and  meaning.  Schemas  are  accommodated  to 
substitute  objects  and  not  to  the  original  signifiers  of  conflict. 


59 


In  defensive  assimilation,  these  relationships  are  reversed. 

The  properties  and  context  of  forms  are  disrupted  in  favor  of  assim¬ 
ilation  to  affectively  important  meanings .  Social  reality  is  sub¬ 
ordinated  to  personal  reality.  Conventionally  appropriate  meanings 
are  ignored  in  deference  to  the  important  Individualized  meanings , 
Objects  are  united  with  one  another.  They  are  used  for  their 
potential  meaning,  on  the  basis  of  imagined  similarity  to  signified 
objects.  At  the  same  time,  minimal  accommodation  to  their  objective 
properties  preserves  the  underlying  sense  of  difference  from  the 
objects  of  the  assimilatory  schema.  Imagined  ends,  therefore,  are 
no  longer  incompatible,  since  the  schemas  are  applied  to  substitute 
objects.  For  instance,  a  child  can  take  liberties  with  his  dolls 
that  he  could  hardly  imagine  taking  with  his  family.  But  when  the 
distinction  between  dolls  and  family  breaks  down,  play  is  Interrupt¬ 
ed,  Again,  in  defensive  assimilation  the  form  is  detached  from  its 
socialized  meanings ,  which  remain  in  the  background,  holding  form 
constant.  Defensive  assimilation  gives  signif ication  to  ideas  of 
union,  and  it  mediates  personal  and  social  reality. 

In  the  pairs  of  stories  discussed  in  the  previous  section,  there 
is  a  movement  toward  one  or  the  other  of  the  defensive  extremes. 

The  transf ormation  from  story  nine  to  story  two  is  essentially  a 
defensive  assimilation.  It  establishes  a  union,  it  disrupts  con¬ 
text,  and  it  utilizes  the  black  figure  primarily  as  a  formless  per¬ 
son,  The  transformation  from  story  three  to  story  eight  Is  a  de¬ 
fensive  accommodation.  It  preserves  context,  it  utilizes  the  black 
figure  primarily  as  a  personless  form,  and  it  mitigates  loss  by 
restructuring  separation.  The  imaged  notion  of  a  racial  dichotomy 
has  the  peculiar  potential  to  aid  defense  in  either  of  its  goals. 


60 


Whether  the  problems  concerns  a  wish  or  a  fear,  black  forms  make 
the  solution  easier. 

The  cost  is  the  violation  of  the  black  person’s  humanity.  He 
becomes  either  the  Human  Being  who  is  not  black,,  or  the  black  animate 
form  who  is  not  human.  One  transf ormation  leads  to  fantasies  of 
blacks  as  loving,  genuine,  beautiful,  and  God-like;  the  other  to 
fantasies  of  blacks  as  animals,  attackers,  filth,  and  property.  If 
interracial  encounters  seem  so  often  to  be  unreasonably  tense  or 
unreasonably  relaxed,  at  the  preconceptual  level  they  quite  liter¬ 
ally  disrupt  equilibrium.  For  defensive  purposes,  whites  find  blacks 
good  to  think  with. 

This  derives  from  the  common  requirements  of  defensive  assim¬ 
ilation  and  defensive  accommodation,  and  the  ability  of  racial  forms 
to  fulfill  these  requirements  maximally.  In  both,  techniques  of  de¬ 
fense,  personal  and  social  reality  must  be  signified.  Since  the 
black  person  is  first  of  all  a  person,  he  may  substitute  for  objects 
of  any  assimilatory  schema  of  persons.  He  is  a  mobile  symbolic 
vehicle.  By  virtue  of  a  single  Imaged  property,  however,  the  black 
person  as  a  signifier  is  always  potentially  different  from  signified 
objects . 

It  will  be  remembered  that  defensive  assimilation  requires  a 
minimal  degree  of  accommodation  to  formal  difference  for  the  pre¬ 
servation  (in  the  TAT)  of  pretense.  In  a  pinch,  the  subject  can 
accommodate  to  a  single  visual  property  (blackness)  and  the  sense 
of  difference  can  easily  be  reinstated.  The  constancy  of  the  ex¬ 
ternal  formal  difference  allows  the  maximal  assimilation  to  that 
form  to  personal  structures  without  reawakening  conflict. 

Defensive  accommodation,  on  the  other  hand,  requires  a  minimal 


61 


degree  of  assimilation,  on  the  basis  of  similarity,  to  preserve 
belief.  The  accommodated  difference  can  never  become  pure  image, 
since  it  would  then  lose  its  general  socialized  meaning  upon  which 
belief  depends.  It  must  become  a  difference  in  groups  of  people. 
That  is,  the  properties  of  the  image-bound  group  must  be  assimilated 
to  at  least  one  human  signified,  which  is  accommodated  to  all  mem¬ 
bers  of  the  group.  This  is  defensive  accommodation  at  its  ma.ximum. 
It  results  in  the  notion  that  all  blacks  are  different  from  whites 
in  a  particular  respect. 

Defensive  assimilation,  carried  to  its  maximum,  results  in  the 
notion  that  a  black  can  be  like  a  white  in  all  respects  but  one. 
Together  these  two  notions—that  there  is  a  single  difference  which 
is  def ined--amount  to  a  preconcept  of  racial  opposition.  By  pre¬ 
serving  this  preconcept  the  mind  can  swing  in  either  direction  with¬ 
out  experiencing  itself  as  regressing  symbolically  and  without  ex¬ 
periencing  intolerable  conflict  between  personal  and  social  reality. 

We  may  formulate  a  hypothesis.  When  the  mind  assimilates  a 
perceptual  group  difference  to  structures  of  conflict,  the  conflict 
will  be  reduced  and  thought  will  be  both  less  intelligent  and  more 
centered.  Assimilation  to  a  structure  of  union  is  defensive  assim¬ 
ilation,  Assimilation  to  a  structure  of  separation  is  defensive 
accommodation.  From  a  perceptual  group  difference  the  notion  of 
racial  opposition  will  emerge. 


62 


Appendix 

Pictures  and  Stories 

Picture  one:  An  old  white  man  is  sitting  at  a  desk.  He  is 
holding  a  pipe  and  his  tie  is  loosened. 

Story  one:  He’s  a  professor  because  uh,  you  can  tell  from  his 
office.  It’s  not  a  very  uh  classy  sort  of  an  office.  He’s  not  a 
businessman  at  all.  He’s  at  ease,  just  uh  sort  of  late  in  the  day 
after  work.  He’s  settling  back  and  taking  in  what  went  on  during 
the  day.  uh  I  guess ,  you  know,  some  sort  of  reflection  before  taking 
off  for  home.  He’s  apparently  not  had  a  very  unusual  day.  He’s 
not  overjoyed  or  distraught  at  anything,  uh  He’s  calm  and  ready  to 
ready  to  end  the  day.  Ready  to  go, 

D.B.:  What  kind  of  things  is  he  reflecting  on? 

S:  I  think  probably  more  about  how  he’s  done.  How  in  his  class  or 
uh  in  his  class,  you  know,  whether  he  was  effective  rather  than 
worrying  about  a  particular  student  or,  I  think  a  problem  of  that 
nature.  It’s  sort  of  an  evaluation  of  his  own  performance  during 
the  day  rather  than  uh  someone  else’s  problem,  uh  A  problem  a  stu¬ 
dent  may  be  having  or  a  colleague. . .It 3 s  just  a  little  bit  of  judg¬ 
ment  at  the  end  of  the  day,  as  to  how  actually  he  did,  how  his 
particular  courses  are  getting  along,  if  he’s  where  he  should  be, 
if  uh  he’s,  if  the  students  are  are  getting  what  he  things  they 
should  be  getting.  If  not,  you  know,  what  in  him  is  the  cause  of 
it  and  what  will  he  be  able  to  do  to  to  remedy  it,  rather  than  you 
know  their  problem.  His  own  sort  of.  Did  I  answer  that? 

D0B0:  Yes,  and  the  outcome? 

S:  (smiles)  The  outcome  will  be  a  sigh,  putting  on  his  coat, 


63 


straightening  out  the.  desk.  You  know.,  I  mean  just  leaving  the  office 
sort  of  action  and  heading  home. 

Story  and  Picture  two:  See  page  22, 

Story  and  Picture  three:  See  page  28. 

Picture  number  four:  a  young  white  man  is  lying  face  down  on 
the  grass  with  his  eyes  closed.  An  old  white  man  in  a  suit  is 
crouched  slightly  toward  him,  several  feet  away. 

Story  number  four:  (finger  tapping)  This  man,  the  old.  man, 
has  urn  come  up  from  far  away  and  can’t  see  very  well  (laughs).  He’s 
uh  sort  of  nearsighted  and  he  being  sort  of  always  conscious  of 
crime  and  and  this  sort  of  thing  that  weren’t  so  rampant  in  the  good 
old  days  (when)  they  didn’t  think  things  he’s  unaccustomed  to,  he 
he’s  wants  to  make  sure  that  this  guy,  who’s  asleep,  is  actually 
asleep  and  ok  rather  than  urn  hurt  in  any  way,  or  uh  rather  than 
overdosed.  So  he’s  approaching  the  guy  and  but  carefully,  so  that 
if  the  guy’s  asleep  he’s  not  going  to  disturb  him,  and  He’ll  realize, 
when  he  gets  close  enough  to  see  him,  you  know,  perceiving  the  guy’s 
breathing,  or  whatever  he  he’s  looking  for  as  a  sign  of  life.  Then, 
he’ll  sort  of  quietly  walk  on  away. 

D.B, :  And  the  guy  on  the  ground? 

S:  uh,  He’s  just  asleep.  In  the  park. 

Picture  five:  A  young  black  man  is  sitting  in  a  chair  with  a 
cigarette  in  one  hand  and  an  opened  letter  and  envelope  dangling 
from  the  other.  There  is  a  desk,  a  wastebasket,  some  books,  and  a 
poster. 

Story  five:  This  guy’s  in  a  familiar  setting.  He’s  in,  he’s 
been  in,  say,  his  college  room  and  he  studied,  uh  and  Just  right 
now  he’s  after  studying  for  a  while.  He’s  come  across  something 


' 

I 

•  V. 

64 


that  is  worth  thinking  about,  is  worth  doing  more  than  just  learn¬ 
ing,  you  know.  It  set  him  off  on  a  tangent  and  he’s  just  sort  of, 
at  the  moment  pondering  whatever  he’s  just  run  across  in  his  book 
and  uh,  will  fairly  soon  go  on  with  the  studying.  So,  it ’  s  nothing 
very  ,  again,  nothing  very  shocking  to  him,  or  it’s  just  something, 
something  that  has  struck  an  interest  in  him  and  urn,  he 3 11  soon  re¬ 
turn  to  studying, 

D.B.:  What  might  it  be? 

S;  uh...I  think  It’s  something  in,  whatever  the  subject,  that  re¬ 
lates  much  more  closely  to  his  own  experience  than  the  rest  of  the 
subject  matter  had.  If  it’s  uh  is  a  novel  or  uh  any  type  of  liter¬ 
ature,  he  has  come  across  a  a  character  who3s  experienced  a  feeling 
that  he’s  very  recently  had  or  is  in  a  situation  very  similar  to 
something  he’s  had,  urn  In  a  more,  if  it's  a  more  scientific  subject, 
he’s  it’s  it’s  a  fact  or  an  interpretation  of  the  world  or  urn  some 
particular  aspect  of  the  world  that  he  recognizes  that  he’s  sort  of 
hand  intuitively  on  his  own  and  never  seen  really  written  and  uh 
articulated  very  well,  It’s  just  It’s  something  that  he  sort  of 
intuitively  agrees  with  and  he!s  thinking  through  this,  It’s  sort 
of  a  good  experience  to  run  across  a  thing  like  that,  He’s  just 
thinking  about  about  uh  the  relation  between  him  and  the  book,  It’s 
closer  than  it  was,  He’s  finding  more  meaning  in  it  than  just  an 
object  for  study. 

Picture  six:  A  young  white  woman  is  leaning  against  a  pillar 
in  front  of  a  brick  building.  It  Is  night. 

Story  six:  (Rubs  hand  through  hair).  This  girl’s  in  a  has 
come  to  a  place  that  she’s  not  familiar  with.  I’ll  say  she’s,  I’ll 
say  she’s  run  away  from  home  just  because  it’s  a  very  institutional 


sort  of  a  building  uh  and  she  looks  out  of  place  in  it.  She’s  got 
a  look  on  her  face  that ’ s .. .lonely  and  sad  and  sort  of  reflective. 

She’s  come  to  this  place  uh  without  a  direction,  without  knowing 
actually  where  she  was  coming,  just  heading  somewhere,  and  has  gotten 
here  as  an  as  an  end  almost  or  or  now  sees  it  as  an  end  and  is  look¬ 
ing  for  somewhere  to  go  on,  somewhere  to,  well,  looking  for  whether 
to  go... to  go  back  or  to  continue  uh  in  whatever  travel,  well  in  her 
travels.  She’s,  It’s  sort  of  the  first  time  she’s  uh  questioned  her 
her  decision  to  leave  her  home.  It’s  the  first  time  she’s  felt  bad¬ 
ly  and  begun  to  wonder  uh  more  about  where  she’s  going  than  where 
she’s  coming  from,  uh  I  think  she’ll  probably  go  on  rather  than 
come  back,  rather  than  return  to  her  home.  It’s  not,  it  she’s  just 
beginning  her  trip.  She’s  not  uh  very  much  beaten  down  by  traveling 
life  and  she  she’s  wondering  where  to  go,  pausing  more  than  ending 
it,  anything  like  that. 

Picture  seven:  A  young  white  man  Is  standing  next  to  an  old 
black  woman.  He  is  turned  toward  her,  while  she  is  facing  away  from 
him.  There  is  a  table  with  a  toaster. 

Story  seven:  There’s  a  young  a  black  kid,  child,  uh  and,  pictured 
here  are  his  mother  and  his  uh  this  sort  of  old  white  kid  that  comes 
and  uh  takes  him  out.  He  and  a  group  of  four  or  five  other  kids  uh 
they  go  do  crafts,  or  the  park,  or  YMCA,  things  like  that.  In  other 
words,  it’s  an  older  kid  working  with  the  younger  kid,  uh  The  young¬ 
er  kid  has  in  his  home  caused  a  disturbance.  He  he’s  caused  some 
trouble  and  uh  while  his  white  friend  is  there  and  while  his  mother 
is  there  together  and  its  just  beginning.  The  white  guy  feels  very 
very  uncomfortable,  not  knowing  his  role.  He’s  uh  he’s  there  he’s 
there  and  has  been  almost  a  father  figure  for  the  kid  but  now  he’s 


„ 


66 


in  the  home  where  the  mother9 s  been  very  definitely  in  control  and 

he  feels  almost  as  if  the  kid  might  be  testing  him,  uh  and  so  uh 

He9s  going  to  wait  and  let  the  mother  take  the  lead,  He’s  not  about 
to  discipline  the  kid  in  his  own  home,  not  about  to  say  anything, 
uh  The  mother 9 s  yeah,  is  used  to  the  kid  making  trouble,  uh  She 

knows  what  he’s  doing  and  will  handle  it  as  soon  as  he  gets  out  of 

hand.  She  9s  just  waiting,  uh,  but  not  nearly  as  uncomfortable  as 
the  other  guy,  as  the  guy.  They’re  both  looking  at  him. 

D.B.;  And  the  outcome? 

S:  (sits  forward)  The  outcome  is  that  the  mother  will  uh,  very 

soon  step  in,  do  what  is  necessary  for  that  particular  child.  She 
will  know  what  what  the  kid  is  up  to,  you  know.  If  he’s  beating  up 
on  his  little  brother,  she’ll  know  how  to  stop  it  and  will  do  so, 
uh,  much  to  the  white  kid’s  great  relief. 

Picture  and  story  eight:  See  page  29,  (Paired  with  three). 
Picture  and  story  nine:  See  page  21.  (paired  with  two). 
Picture  ten:  An  old  black  man  in  shirt  and  tie  is  sitting  on 
the  edge  of  a  chair.  He  is  slightly  bent  and  has  a  soda  in  his 
hand.  Behind  him  is  a  vending  maching.  (Paired  with  one). 

Story  ten:  This  is  a  guy  uh  taking  a  break  from  his  work,  not 
at  the  end  of  the  day,  but  in  the  middle  of  the  day,  uh  say  mid¬ 
morning,  and  has  a  specific  problem  on  his  mind,  uh  that’s  bothering 
him  pretty  seriously,  not  like  the  professor  in  the  first  picture 
who’s  just  evaluating  his  whole  day.  This  guy’s  got  a  particular 
thing  on  his  mind  that’s  troubling  him.  uh  Again  I’ll  say  he’s  a 
professor,  and  in  this  instance  it’s  a  class  that  just  immediately 
preceeded  him  coming  out  and  taking  his  break,  uh  It’s  a  small  class 
and  it  it’s  just  upset  him  very  much  since  he  knows  the  students  on 


67 


an  individual  basis  and  can  judge ,  therefore,  how  much  work  they’re 
doing  and  and  the  commitment  they  have  to  the  course,  that  that  They 
don’t  have  the  commitment  he  wants  and  uh  when  that’s  so  it’s  use¬ 
less  to  try  and  be  a  good  teacher  because,  you  know,  even  if  you 
are  a  good  teacher  they’re  not  going  to  benefit  that  much  from  it 
unless  they  are  are  good  students  and  he’s  finding  that  the  students 
are  not  in  this  particular  class  giving  what  he  wants,  and  uh  so 
that  uh  He’ll  come  out  of  it  with  a  resolve  to  uh  improve  his  own 
teaching  hoping  that  it  will  inspire  the  uh  the  uh  students,  rather 
than  to  sort  of  harangue  them  about  you  got  to  start  reading  the 
books.  He  will  uh  come  out  of  it  hoping  to  be  able  to  inspire  them 
to  get  more  into  that  course.  That’s  all. 

Picture  eleven:  A  young  white  man  is  sitting  in  a  chair  and 
holding  a  telephone  receiver  several  inches  off  its  base.  His  foot 
is  on  a  book,  and  there  are  bookshelves  behind  him.  (Paired  with 
five ) • 

Story  eleven:  This  guy  is  uh  has  spent  the  last  couple  hours 
trying  to  study  or  trying  to  read,  uh  but  he’s  got  a  personal  pro¬ 
blem  on  his  mind,  I  mean  a  uh  a  problem  about  his  relationship  with 
with  a  girl,  and  uh  it’s  just  really  thwarting  every  effort  he  tries, 
you  know,  to  study  that  he  makes.  He  can’t  can’t  concentrate  but 
he’s  been  trying  because,  one,  he  just  doesn’t  want  to  uh  let  the 
girl  get  him  down  right  now,  and  two  he  he’s  feels  that  he  should 
be  studying  this  stuff  for  the  next  day’s  class,  uh  But  finally  with 
uh,  he’s  sort  of  dropped  his  book  uh  here  and  has  decided  to  go  ahead 
and  call  the  girl  to  work  out  what  it  is,  what  the  problem  is,  and 
uh,  by  doing  so  sort  of  clear  his  head,  so  he  can  go  on  and  study. 

He  will,  well  uh,  depending  on  the  phone  call.  It  it  turns  out  to 


. 


68 


be  a  bad  call  he  might  have  to  uh  go  out  uh  see  the  person,,  or  or 
go  out  on  his  own,  sort  of  walk  around,  that  sort  of  thing,  uh  If 
things  work  out  the  way  he  hopes,  then  he 3 11  be  able  to  just  settle 
back  and  do  that  studying  that  he  wasn’t  able  to  do  before,  (laughs) 
I  can’t  say  which  it  would  be,  uh. 

Picture  twelve:  A  young  white  man  with  closed  eyes  is  lying 
on  his  back  on  a  couch.  An  old  black  man  in  a  suit  is  standing  over 
him  with  one  arm  outstretched.  (Paired  with  four). 

Story  twelve:  (long  pause)  Two  uh  colleagues,  two  friends, 
two  teachers,  that  know  each  other  uh  and  are  friends,  uh  The  one 
lying  down  has  uh  well  he’s  resting.  He’s  not  asleep,  but  he’s  just 
resting  after  delivering  a  lecture  and  he’s  tired  of  standing  up 
(laughs).  The  other  comes  in,  that  has  come  in,  and  uh  sort  of 
feeling  pretty  good  uh  for  whatever  reason,  his  own  reason,  either 
he  had  a  good  class  or  life  looked  good  in  the  morning  or  whatever, 
and  comes  in,  sort  of  going  to  slap  the  guy  on  the  back,  uh  yeah  uh, 
just  getting  ready  for  a  greeting  here.  He’s  just  walked  in  and 
they  are  good  friends  and  he’s  just  gonna  sort  of  to  refresh  the 
guy  that’s  lying  down.  It’s  just  a  very  happy  scene.  Both  will  go 
on  to  uh  finish  the  day  in  good  spirits. 

Picture  thirteen:  A  young  black  woman  is  sitting  on  the  steps 
in  front  of  a  brick  building.  It  is  night.  (Paired  with  six). 

Story  thirteen:  It’s  a  woman  uh  outside  of  a  building  in  which 
she  works,  waiting  for  a  ride,  uh  to  be  picked  up  by  uh.  She’s 
married  uh,  but  her  husband’s  not  picking  her  up.  It’s  a  a  friend 
of,  a  female  type  friend  of  hers,  who’s  also  married,  uh  And  both 
are  sort  of  upset  (looks  away  from  the  picture)  because  their 
husbands  are  good  friends  and  are  always  going  out  together  without 


69 


uh  uh  paying  a  whole  lot  of  attention  to  to  the  wives,  the  wives  feel# 
And  so  they  they’re  together  fairly  often  in  this  situation  where 
the  husbands  are  are  out#  un  and  so  She’s  she’s  waiting  and  she’ll 
get  picked  up  and  they’ll  exchange,  you  know,  hellos  and  little 
gossips  and  such  things  and  then  uh  possibly  get  together  uh  for  a 
for  a  little  while  before  each  goes  home  to  uh,  to  take  care  of  the 
kids#  They  both  have  very  young  children#  They  can't,  they  would 
like  to  be  able  to  spend  the  evening  and  the  night  together,  doing 
something  like  the  husbands  can,  but  they  have  to  care  for  the  kids, 
and  they  don’t  like  it.  She’s  very  sad,  so  she’ll  go  and  commiser¬ 
ate  for  a  little  while  with  her  friend.  You  know. 

Picture  fourteen:  A  young  white  man  is  standing  several  feet 
from  an  old  white  woman,  who  is  sitting  in  a  chair.  He  is  facing 
her,  while  she  is  looking  into  the  distance.  There  is  a  newspaper 
on  the  table  beside  her,  (Paired  with  seven). 

Story  fourteen:  A  son  and  his  mother,  uh  The  son  uptight  over 
a  decision  he  wants  to  make,  concerning  his  future  that’s  very 
important  to  him,  and  that  his  mother  disapproves  of#  uh  He’s  pretty 
much  decided  that  he  wants  to  uh  drop  out  of  law  school  and  she  has 
lived  most  of  her  life  with  a  very  distinguished  lawyer  and  can't 
understand  her  son’s  wishes  to  uh  not  continue  and  be  a  lawyer  him¬ 
self,  because  they’re  new  to  him,  or  uh  they’re  newly  expressed  by 
him.  Although  it  means  a  great  deal  to  him  and  has  been  sort  of 
growing  in  his  mind  for  a  while,  he  never  uh  has  never  expressed 
much  of  it  to  her  because  they’re  simply  not  very  close,  uh  If  he 
was  trying  to  decide  whether  to  drop  out  of  law  school  or  not  his 
mother  would  not  be  one  of  the  first  people  he’s  turn  to  to  talk 
about  it  with#  And  so  she  doesn’t  uh  doesn’t  approve  at  all  and  is 


70 


upset  that  he’s  going  to  go  ahead  and  do  it,  upset  that  he  uh  uh 
must  incur  his  mother 9 s  disapproval  uh  which  means.  It  doesn’t 
mean  anything  like  uh  they 3 re  going  to  disown  him  or  anything  of 
that  nature,  It!s  not  a  very  violent  scene  at  all,  uh  They9 re  both 
just  upset  with  each  other  uh  each  other’s  attitude.  He  will  go 
ahead  and  do  it  uh.  She  won’t  approve,  and  it’ll  just  be  one  more 
strain  or  uh  one  more.  They’ll  just  be  standing  another  foot  apart. 
They  just,  just  one  more  incident  serving  to  uh  undo  any  closeness 
they’ve  had  uh  which  means  that  he’ll  be  even  pushed  more  before  he’ll 
tell  her  anything  in  the  future.,, But  he’ll  go  ahead  and  do  it. 


* 


71 


Bibliography 

Ackerman  and  Jahoda.  "The  Dynamic  Basis  of  Anti-Semitism, "  The 
Psychoanalytic  Quarterly,  XVII  (1948),  240-260. 

Allport,  Gordon.  The  Nature  of  Prejudice .  Anchor  Books,  Garden 
City,  New  Jersey,  1958. 

Bird,  Brian.  "A  Consideration  of  the  Etiology  of  Prejudice," 

Journal  of  the  American  Psychoanalytic  Association.  V  (1957), 

490-513. 

Coles,  Robert.  Children  of  Crisis .  Little,  Brown,  and  Company, 
Boston  and  Toronto,  1964. 

Comer,  James  P,  "White  Racism:  Its  Root,  Form,  and  Function," 
American  Journal  of  Psychiatry.  126, no.  6  (1969),  70-74. 

Hamilton,  James,  "Some  Dynamics  of  Anti-Negro  Prejudice," 
Psychoanalytic  Review,  LIII  (19 66),  5-15« 

Jordan,  Winthrop.  White  over  Black,  Penguin  Books,  Inc.,  Baltimore, 
Maryland,  19 68. 

Kovel,  Joel,  White  Racism ,  A  Psychohistory.  Pantheon  Books , 

New  York,  1970, 

Kubie,  Lawrence,  "The  Ontogeny  of  Racial  Prejudice,"  J ournal  of 
Nervous  and  Mental  Diseases .  CXLI  (1965),  p.  265, 

Piaget,  Jean.  Play,  Dreams,  and  Imitation  in  Childhood.  Translated 
by  G .  Gattegno  and  F.M.  Hodgson,  The  Norton  Library,  W.W.  Norton 
and  Company,  Inc.,  New  York,  1962. 

Pinderhugh.es ,  C.A.  Lecture  before  a  joint  meeting  of  the  American 

Psychiatric  Association  and  American  Psychoanalytic  Association, 
Washington,  D.C.,  1971° 


t 


\ 


Pinderhughes ,  C . A 


72 


"Understanding  Black  Power:  Processes  and 
Proposals,  "  The  American  J ournal  of  Psychiatry ,  125  (1969), 
1552-1557. 

Rogers,  Terry.  "The  Evolution  of  an  Active  Anti-Negro  Racist." 

The  Psychoanalytic  Study  of  Society ,  I  {i960),  243-247® 
Sterba,  Richard.  "Some  Psychological  Factors  in  Negro  Race  Hatred 
and  in  Anti-Negro  Riots,"  Psychoanalysis  and  the  Social 
Sciences,  I  (1947),  411-427. 

West,  L.J.  "The  Psychobiology  of  Racial  Violence,"  The  Archives 
of  General  Psychiatry.  XVI  (1967),  645-651® 

Zilboorg.  "The  Psychopathology  of  Social  Prejudice,"  The  Psycho¬ 
analytic  Quarterly,  XVI  (1947),  303-324, 


.  . 

. 


, 


YALE  UNIVERSITY  LIBRARY 


3  9002  0 


496  2584 


Date  Due 


All  books  are  subject  to  recall  after  two  weeks. 


oQ^ 


YALE  MEDICAL  LIBRARY 


Manuscript  Theses 


Unpublished  theses  submitted  for  the  Master's  and  Doctor's  degrees  and 
deposited  in  the  Yale  Medical  Library  are  to  be  used  only  with  due  regard  to  the 
rights  of  the  authors.  Bibliographical  references  may  be  noted,  but  passages 
must  not  be  copied  without  permission  of  the  authors,  and  without  proper  credit 
being  given  in  subsequent  written  or  published  work. 

This  thesis  by  has  been 

used  by  the  following  persons,  whose  signatures  attest  their  acceptance  of  the 
above  restrictions. 


NAME  AND  ADDRESS 


DATE