Skip to main content

Full text of "The time-relations of mental phenomena"

See other formats


I 


tfact  an&  UbcotT  papers. 


NUMBER  VI. 
THE 

TIME-RELATIONS  OF  MENTAL  PHENOMENA 

BY  PROF.  JOSEPH  JASTROW 


fact  an£>  3beore  papers. 


THE  TIME-RELATIONS  OF 
MENTAL  PHENOMENA 


BY 

JOSEPH  JASTROW, 

Professor  of  Psychology  at  the   University  of  Wisconsin. 


NEW    YORK : 

N.  D.  C.  HODGES,  PUBLISHER 

47  LAFAYETTE  PLACE 

1890 


THE    TIME-KELATIONS    OF 
MENTAL   PHENOMENA. 


THE  study  of  the  time  relations  of  mental  phenomena  is 
important  from  several  points  of  view:  it  serves  as  an  index 
of  mental  complexity,  giving  the  sanction  of  objective 
demonstration  to  the  results  of  subjective  observation ;  it 
indicates  a  mode  of  analysis  of  the  simpler  mental  acts,  as 
well  as  the  relation  of  these  laboratory  products  to  the  pro- 
cesses of  daily  life;  it  demonstrates  the  close  inter-relation 
of  psychological  with  physiological  facts,  an  analysis  of  the 
former  being  indispensable  to  the  right  comprehension  of  the 
latter;  it  suggests  means  of  lightening  and  shortening  men- 
tal operations,  and  thus  offers  a  mode  of  improving  educa- 
tional methods;  and  it  promises  in  various  directions  to 
deepen  and  widen  our  knowledge  of  those  processes  by  the 
complication  and  elaboration  of  which  our  mental  life  is  so 
wonderfully  built  up.  It  is  only  within  very  recent  years 
that  this  department  of  research  has  been  cultivated  ;  and  it 
is  natural  that  the  results  of  different  workers,  involving 
variations  in  method  and  design,  should  show  points  of 


2  THE  TIME-RELATIONS   OF 

difference.  In  spite  of  these  it  seems  possible  to  present  a 
systematic  sketch  of  what  has  been  don?,  with  due  reference 
to  the  ultimate  goal  as  well  as  to  the  many  gaps  still  to  be 
filled.  It  is  with  the  object  of  furnishing  such  a  general 
view  that  the  following  exposition  has  been  attempted. 
Rate  of  Nervous  Impulses. 

While  it  follows,  as  a  very  natural  consequence  of  the 
modern  view  of  the  relation  between  body  and  mind,  that 
mental  processes,  however  simple,  should  occupy  time,  it 
must  be  remembered  that  the  very  opposite  opinion  has 
been  held  by  serious  thinkers.  It  has  been  argued  as  a  proof 
of  the  immateriality  of  thought  that  its  operations  were  out 
of  relation  to  time,  and  the  expression  "quick  as  thought" 
has  come  to  indicate  a  maximum  -of  speed.  It  being  estab- 
lished thr-t  so  comparatively  simple  a  process  as  sensation 
involves  the  passage  of  an  impulse  along  nerve-fibres,  it  is 
plain  that  the  rate  of  travelling  of  this  impulse  sets  a  limit 
to  the  time  of  the  entire  process,  as  well  as  of  all  more  com- 
plicated mental  operations  in  which  sensations  are  involved. 
The  physiologist  Johannes  Muller,  writing  in  1844,  despaired 
of  our  ever  being  able  to  measure  the  time  of  so  excessively 
rapid  and  short  a  movement;  but  before  the  close  of  the  same 
decade,  Helmholtz  measured  the  rate  in  the  nerve  of  the  frog, 
finding  it  to  be  about  86  feet  per  second.  Though  somewhat 
greater  in  man.  110  feet  per  second,  this  movement  is  ex- 
tremely slow  compared  with  the  velocity  of  light  or  even 
sound:  indeed,  it  is  only  slightly  faster  than  the  fastest  ex- 
press train. 

Mi'iller  writes:  '"  We  shall  probably  never  secure  the  means 
of  ascertaining  the  speed  of  nerve  activity,  because  we  lack 
the  comparison  of  enormous  distances  from  which  the  speed 
of  a  movement,  in  this  respect  analogous  to  light,  could  be 
calculated;"  and  again:  "The  time  in  which  a  sensation 
proceeds  from  the  periphery  to  the  brain  and  the  spinal  cord, 
and  is  followed  by  a  re-action  at  the  periphery  by  means  of 
muscular  contractions,  is  infinitely  small  and  immeasura- 


MENTAL   PHENOMENA.  3 

ble."  It  is  interesting  to  note  how  very  crude  were  the  con- 
ceptions of  the  older  physiologists  upon  this  point.  Haller 
(1762)  tells  us  of  one  who,  following  the  view  that  the  ner- 
vous impulse  was  a  fluid,  and  its  action  analagous  to  that  of 
the  blood,  fourd  the  "  nerve  tubes"  of  the  heart  to  be  2,880 
million  times  as  narrow  as  the  aorta,  and  concluded  that  the 
nervous  impulse  travelled  proportionately  faster  than  the 
blood,  thus  making  its  rate  57,600  million  feet  per  second. 
Haller  himself  measured  the  maximum  rapidity  of  short 
rhythmical  movements,  and  (falsely),  assuming  that  the  im- 
pulse travelled  to  and  from  muscle  and  brain  between  each 
contraction,  found  an  (accidentally  not  very  erroneous) 
speed  of  9,000  feet  per  second.  The  method  introduced  by 
Helmholtz,  and  improved  by  himself  and  others,  consists  in 
excising  a  muscle  with  a  long  stretch  of  nerve  attached, 
and  connecting  the  muscle  with  a  lever,  so  that  every  con- 
traction of  it  is  registered  upon  the  quickly  moving  surface 
of  a  revolving  drum  or  a  swinging  pendulum.  By  electrically 
stimulating  the  nerve  first  at  a  point  near  to  and  then  at  a 
point  far  off  from  the  muscle,  two  curves  are  recorded,  the 
latter  of  which  is  found  to  leave  the  base  line  a  trifle  after 
the  former.  A  tuning-fork  writes  its  vibrations  beneath 
these  records,  and  enables  us  to  measure  how  much  later 
the  second  contraction  began,  while  the  distance  travelled 
in  this  time  is  that  between  the  two  points  of  stimula- 
tion on  the  nerve.  It  has  been  attempted  to  measure  this 
rate  in  man  by  having  the  subject  re-act  once  to  a  stimulus 
applied  to  the  foot,  and  again  to  a  stimulus  at  the  hip,  or 
some  point  nearer  the  spinal  cord,  and  counting  the  differ- 
ence in  time  as  due  to  the  difference  in  length  of  nerve 
traversed.  While  the  method  is  necessarily  inaccurate,  and 
other  factors  contribute  to  the  difference  in  time,  the  majority 
of  the  determinations  indicate  a  rate  of  between  30  and  40 
metres  (100  to  130  feet)  per  second.  These  determinations 
apply  to  sensory  nerves:  for  the  motor  nerves  of  man,  Helm- 
holtz has  found,  by  a  method  closely  similar  to  that  employed 
upon  the  frog,  a  rate  of  110  teet  per  second.  The  most  in- 
fluential of  the  conditions  affecting  this  rate  is  temperature: 
cold  decreases  and  heat  increases  it.  the  extremes  of  varia- 
tion being  30  to  90  metres.  Under  normal  conditions  it 
seems  fait-  to  regard  the  rate  for  both  motor  and  sensory 
nerves  of  man  as  about  110  feet  per  second. 


THE   TIME-RELATIONS   OF 

Analysis  of  Re-actions. 

A  great  variety  of  actions  may  be  viewed  as  responses  to 
stimuli.  There  is  a  flash  of  light,  and  we  wink;  a  burning 
cinder  falls  upon  the  hand,  and  we  draw  it  away;  a  bell 
rings,  and  the  engineer  starts  his  train,  or  the  servant  opens 
the  door,  or  we  go  down  to  dinner;  the  clock  strikes,  and 
we  stop  work,  or  go  to  meet  an  appointment.  Again,  in 
such  an  occupation  as  copying,  every  letter  or  word  seen 
acts  as  a  stimulus,  to  which  the  written  letter  or  word  is  the 
response;  in  piano  playing,  and  the  guidance  of  complicated 
machinery,  we  see  more  elaborate  instances  of  similar  pro- 
cesses. The  printer  distributing  "pi."  the  post-office  clerk 
sorting  the  mails,  are  illustrations  of  quick  forms  of  re-action, 
in  which  the  different  letters  of  the  alphabet  or  the  different 
addresses  of  the  mail  matter  act  as  the  stimuli,  and  the  plac- 
ing them  in  their  appropriate  places  follows  as  the  response. 
In  many  games,  such  as  tennis  or  cricket,  the  various  ways 
in  which  the  ball  is  seen  to  come  to  the  striker  ai'e  the 
stimuli,  for  each  variation  of  which  there  is  a  precise  and 
complex  form  of  response  in  the  mode  of  returning  the  ball. 
In  military  drill  the  various  words  of  command  are  the 
stimuli,  and  the  actions  thus  induced  the  responses;  and  such 
illustrations  could  be  multiplied  indefinitely.  In  all  these 
actions  the  time-relations  are  more  or  less  definite  and  im- 
portant, but  a  useful  study  of  them  presupposes  a  careful 
and  systematic  analysis  of  the  processes  therein  involved. 
We  recognize  that  certain  of  the  above  actions  are  more 
complicated  than  others,  and  we  must  inquire  in  what  this 
complication  consists.  In  the  process  as  usually  presented 
the  nature  of  the  re-action  depends  upon  the  nature  of  the 
stimulus,  a  variation  in  the  one  being  concomitant  with  a 
variation  in  the  other.  The  piano  player,  seeing  a  certain 
mark  on  the  page,  strikes  a  certain  key  on  the  key-board, 
but  strikes  a  different  key  if  this  mark  be  differently  placed ; 
the  soldier  varies  his  movement  according  to  the  word  of 


MENTAL  PHENOMENA.  5 

command,  aud  so  on  with  most  of  the  others.  All  such 
actions  in volve  at  least  three  processes:  (1)  the  recognition 
of  the  sense  impression,  (2)  the  performance  of  the  appro- 
priate action,  and  (3)  the  association  of  the  one  with  the 
other.  The  recognition  involves  the  appreciation  of  the 
presence  together  with  the  appreciation  of  the  nature  of  the 
sense-impression;  and  the  movement  involves  the  contrac- 
tion of  muscles  together  with  the  initiation  of  the  impulse. 
We  obtain  the  simplest  form  of  re-action  by  limiting  the 
stimulus  to  a  single  definite  one,  and  having  one  and  the 
same  response  irrespective  of  the  nature  of  the  stimulus. 
The  subject  expects  the  stimulus  the  nature  of  which  he 
knows,  and  is  ready  to  signal,  by  a  simple  movement  agreed 
upon  in  advance,  merely  that  the  impression  has  bsen  re- 
ceived. This  we  shall  speak  of  as  a  "simple  re-action."  It 
occurs  whenever  a  certain  sense-impression  is  agreed  upon 
as  a  signal  for  the  execution  of  a  simple  movement.  Tlie 
lime-keeper  pressing  the  spring  of  the  stop-watch,  or  the 
racer  starting  off  as  soon  as  the  pistol  is  fired  or  the  word 
is  given,  are  instances  of  simple  re-actions.  It  should  be 
noted  that  the  simplicity  of  the  act  refers  primarily  to  the 
subject's  fore-knowledge  of  what  is  to  occur;  the  nature  of 
the  sense-impression,  as  of  the  motion,  is  known  in  advance, 
the  association  between  the  two  being  in  the  main  artificial. 
Inasmuch  as  the  more  elaborate  mental  processes  involve 
those  of  the  simple  re-action,  our  first  step  must  be  to  deter- 
mine its  elements  and  their  time-relations. 

The  Elements  of  a  Simple  Re  action. 

The  several  elements  of  a  simple  re-action  have  been 
variously  analyzed  by  different  observers,  but  all  recognize 
the  physiological  and  the  specially  psychological  portions 
of  the  process.  The  physiological  time-elements  include, 
(a)  the  time  for  the  sense-organ  to  respond  to  an  impression, 
i.e.,  to  overcome  its  inertia;  (b)  the  time  for  the  passage  of 
the  impulse  inward  along  nerves  (and  spinal  cord),  with  the 


6  THE   TIME-RELATIONS  OF 

various  delays  whenever  the  impulse  eaters  or  passes  through 
ceils;  (c)  the  return  passage  of  the  motor  impulse  from  the 
brain  to  (spinal  chord  and)  nerve  and  muscle;  and  (d)  the 
time  for  the  contraction  of  the  muscle.  The  time  thus  left 
unaccounted  for  is  that  taken  up  by  the  psychological  pro- 
cess, the  transformation  of  the  sensory  into  the  motor  im- 
pulse,— a  process  taking  place  in  the  brain,  but  as  to  the 
precise  nature  of  which  we  have  no  definite  information. 
The  separate  determination  of  each  of  the  physiological  fac- 
tors enables  us  to  find  approximately  the  duration  of  the 
central  process  As  a  sufficiently  typical  case  we  may  accept 
the  estimate  of  Cattell,  that,  in  re-acting  to  a  light  by  press- 
ing the  key  with  the  finger,  the  time  needed  by  the  impulse 
to  travel  from  eye  to  brain  and  from  brain  to  spinal  cord 
and  finger  is  about  507 ;*  the  latent  time  in  the  muscle,  dur- 
ing which  it  overcomes  its  inertia,  is  judged  from  experi- 
ments upon  the  frog  to  be  about  5(7  to  10(7;  and  experiment 
gives  a  value  of  15(7  to  20(7  for  stimulating  the  retina  and 
initiating  the  impulse.  As  the  entire  re-action  occupied 
about  150(7  we  conclude  that  in  this  case  the  physiological 
and  the  psychological  portions  of  the  process  occupy  about 
equal  times.  One  may  obtain  a  fair  notion  of  the  rate  of 
these  processes  by  the  following  simple  experiment.  A 
score  or  so  of  persons  form  a  chain  by  joining  hands,  and  at  a 
given  signal  a  certain  member  of  the  group  sharply  presses 
the  hand  of  Ids  neighbor,  who  in  turn  imparts  the  pressure 
as  quickly  as  possible  to  his  neighbor,  and  so  on  until  the 
impression  has  gone  the  rounds.  An  outsider  keeps  the 
time  (which  may  be  done  with  sufficient  accuracy  by  count- 
ing the  ticks  of  a  watch,  usually  fifths  of  a  second)  from  the 
moment  of  giving  the  signal  to  start  to  the  moment  of  re- 
ceiving the  signal  from  the  last  member  of  the  group  that 
the  impression  has  been  circulated.  The  entire  time  divided 
by  the  number  of  persons  in  the  group  (or  better,  by  that 

1  The  sign  <r  indicates  one  one-thousandth  of  a  second. 


MENTAL   PHENOMENA.  7 

number  plus  two  to  include  the  re-actions  at  starting  and 
stopping)  gives  an  average  simple  re-action  time,  which, 
though  long  at  first,  is  reduced  after  a  lii/cle  practice  to  a 
sixth  or  a  seventh  of  a  second.  On  this  basis  one  may  cal- 
culate that  if  a  number  of  men,  stretching  out  their  arms 
and  grasping  one  another's  hands,  were  stationed  in  a 
straight  line,  it  would  take  three  minutes  to  send  a  message  in 
the  manner  just  described  along  a  mile  of  this  human  telegraph. 

(a)  The  inertia  of  sense-organs  has  been  variously  deter- 
mined. One  method  measures  how  closely  impressions  may 
follow  one  another  without  fusing.  The  time  thus  measured 
is  the  minimum  time  during  which  the  sense-organ  may  he 
stimulated  and  recover  sufficiently  to  receive  a  second  stim- 
ulation. This  process  thus  includes  something  more  than 
the  one  we  desire  to  measure,  and  may  perhaps  be  regarded 
as  furnishing  a  maximum  time  of  the  sensory  inertia.  Here 
again  various  circumstances  influence  the  determinations, 
the  chief  ones  being  the  sense-organ  in  question  and  the 
clearness  and  intensity  of  the  impression.  Sectors  of  black 
and  white  upon  a  disc  revolving  in  daylight  at  the  rate  of 
about  25  times  a  second  fuse  into  a  uniform  gray,  making 
the  inertia  of  the  retina  under  these  conditions  about  40(7. 
In  weak  light  (moonlight)  the  time  lengthens  to  about  lOOff. 
The  same  experiment  has  been  made  with  sectors  of  different 
colors,  with  the  disc  stationary  and  the  light  reflected  from 
a  rotating  mirror,  with  a  vibrating  point  of  light;  and,  while 
all  these  variations  somewhat  affect  the  result,  the  majority 
of  the  determinations  indicate  a  fusion  at  30  to  40  impressions 
a  second,  or  a  duration  of  S3ff  to  25O".  For  sound,  different 
observers  have  chosen  different  points  for  measurement. 
The  slowest  rate  of  impressions  fusing  into  a  musical  sound 
has  been  fixed  at  between  30  and  40  per  second  ;  but  Helmholtz 
has  shown  that  the  interi'erence  of  sound-waves  perceptible 
as  beats  does  not  escape  detection  when  recurring  as  rapidly 
as  132  per  second.  For  non-musical  sounds,  such  as  electric 
clicks,  a  still  higher  rate  has  been  found.  In  touch  we  dis- 
tinguish differences  of  feeling  when  impressions  are  rapid 
enough  to  fuse  but  not  rapid  enough  to  fuse  perfectly.  The 
smoothness  of  a  polished  surface  is  not  obtained  until  the 
impressions  occur  480  to  640  times  a  second  (Valentin).  For 
taste  and  smell  the  period,  though  not  accurately  determined, 


THE   TIME-RELATIONS  OP 

is  undoubtedly  very  long.  Here  the  time  needed  to  reach 
the  somewhat  concealed  sense-organs  is  considerable,  and  the 
chemical  processes  involved  are  relatively  slow  in  action. 
The  influence  of  the  mode  of  activity  of  the  sense-organ  upon 
its  period  of  stimulation  is  further  illustrated  in  the  long 
inertia  period  of  the  probably  chemical  action  of  vision  com- 
pared with  the  short  period  of  the  mechanical  senses  of  hear- 
ing and  touch.  This  view  is  also  supported  by  the  fact  that 
the  period  for  the  retina  is  shortened  if  the  eye  be  directly 
stimulated  electrically.  Another  mode  of  experimenting 
consists  in  applying  a  stimulus  for  the  minimum  time  during 
which  it  can  be  recognized.  The  time  thus  measured  will  be 
shorter  than  the  other,  for  it  tells  us  only  how  long  is  needed 
for  initiating  the  process  of  recognition  sufficiently  to  have 
it  continue  to  completion  (probably  after  the  stimulus  ceases). 
This  is  indeed  a  surprisingly  short  time.  Cattell  and  San- 
ford  independently  found  that  a  color  or  a  letter  could  be 
recognized  when  visible  for  only  from  Iff  to  5ff,  while  less 
recently  Baxt  recognized  6  to  7  letters  when  exposed  for  only 
5<7.  Others  have  calculated  that  the  maximum  effect  of  an 
impression  is  not  reached  until  from  50 ff  to  150(7,  but  these 
determinations  seem  to  involve  some  mental  process  of  recog- 
nition. Whether  or  not  some  such  process  of  recognition  is 
involved  is  not  quite  clear.  Unless  specially  prevented,  the 
recognition  will  take  place  on  the  basis  of  the  after-image,  a 
few  thousandths  of  a  second  being  sufficient  to  initiate  the 
process.  By  following  the  impression  by  a  strong  flash  of 
light,  and  thus  nullifying  the  afterimage,  Baxt  found  a 
longer  time  needed  to  recognize  a  more  complicated  impres- 
sion. Within  10(7  to  15(7,  one  letter :  within  24(7,  three  letters ; 
within  34(7,  four  letters,  could  be  recognized.  This  only 
partially  excludes  the  effect  of  the  after-image,  so  that  per- 
haps the  results  with  complicated  impressions  are  minimum 
"recognition  times,"  and  those  with  simple  impressions 
''  inertia  times.''  Another  method,  that  of  Exner,  is  similar 
to  the  method  of  fusion.  It  consists  in  finding  how  closely 
two  impressions,  stimulating  slightly  different  portions  of 
the  sense-organ,  may  follow  one  another  and  yet  be  recog- 
nized as  successive.  Optical  impressions  were  so  recognized 
when  falling  at  an  interval  of  44(7  on  two  points  of  the  retina 
near  the  centre  .011  millimetres  apart,  a  longer  time  being 
necessary  if  the  points  are  away  from  the  centre  of  the  ret- 
ina, It  seems  probable  that  this  process  is  more  complicated 


MENTAL  PHENOMENA.  9 

than  the  one  we  are  attempting  to  study.  While  the  data 
thus  at  our  disposal  do  not  allow  us  to  fix  accurately  the 
time  of  sensory  inertia,  the  estimate  provisionally  accepted 
in  the  text  cannot  be  far  from  the  truth,  being  rather  over 
than  under  estimated.  The  methods  of  measuring  the  rate 
of  nervous  impulses  (6)  and  (c)  have  already  been  described. 
The  inertia  of  the  muscle  and  the  time  of  its  contraction  are 
determined  upon  the  same  apparatus  by  observing  how  much 
;:fter  the  shock  is  given  the  curve  leaves  the  base-line. 

Reflex,  Automatic,  and  Voluntary  Re-actions. 
The  term  "  re-action  "  as  here  used  is  not  intended  to  in- 
clude all  responses  to  stimuli.  The  above  instanced  forms 
of  re  actions  present  various  grades  of  naturalness,  utility, 
and  habituation;  that  is,  the  association  between  motion  and 
stimulus  has  by  practice  become  more  or  less  close  and  easy. 
Copying,  for  example,  may  become  so  entirely  automatic 
that  it  runs  on  of  itself  without  the  need  of  renewed  voli- 
tional effort.  The  actions  recognized  as  reflex  take  place  in 
spile  of  all  volition.  The  re-actions  here  considered  are 
limited  to  those  requiring  some  degree  of  voluntary  effort  for 
their  execution,  though  this  may  be  almost  indefinitely  re- 
duced by  practice.  The  reflex  act  takes  very  much  less  time 
for  its  execution  than  the  voluntary:  the  time  for  winking 
has  been  determined  by  Exner  to  be  something  over  50(7. 
In  other  words,  it  takes  about  three  times  as  long  to  signal 
by  a  voluntary  closure  of  the  eye-lid  that  an  impression  has 
been  received  as  to  perform  the  same  act  reflexly  when 
the  eye  is  threatened.  The  utility  of  this  quick  action  for 
the  protection  of  the  eye  is  evident;  and  other  useful  re-ac- 
tions, such  as  those  of  flight  and  escape  in  timid  animals, 
seem  to  be  of  a  similar  nature.  The  quick  movements  of 
defence  when  attacked,  of  regaining  one's  balance  when  slip- 
ping, are  so  immediately  useful  and  so  well  inculcated  in 
the  organism  as  often  to  surprise  us  by  their  quickness. 
Most  of  these  actions  can  also  be  performed  voluntarily,  but 
neither  so  well  nor  so  quickly;  it  is  therefore  difficult  to 


10  THE   TIME-RELATIONS  OF 

subject  them  to  experiment.  As  already  indicated,  in  the 
oi'dinary  re-action  there  is  little  naturalness  in  the  connec- 
tion of  stimulus  and  movement,  the  same  type  of  movement 
being1  used  for  all.  The  experienced  re-actor  becomes  accus- 
tomed to  signal  by  the  finger  movement  that  the  operation 
required  of  him  has  been  accomplished,  but  hardly  associ- 
ates this  movement  with  any  particular  stimulus. 

It  is  perhaps  well  to  add  that  the  great  saving  of  time  in 
actions  that  have  become  automatic  (such  as  is  seen  in  the 
experienced  piano  player  or  post-office  clerk  as  compared 
with  the  beginner),  is  in  great  part  due  to  the  increased 
facility  of  doing  several  things  at  once  and  not  serially,  a 
factor  that  enters  only  in  a  small  degree  into  the  simple  re- 
action. The  processes  we  should  be  most  interested  in 
measuring  are  those  most  closely  approaching  the  operations 
of  daily  thought,  so  that  the  inference  from  experiment  to 
practice  shall  be  as  direct  as  possible.  This,  however,  it  is 
difficult  to  do,  because  every-day  mental  processes  do  not 
present  the  simplicity  of  conditions  required  by  experiment. 
Accordingly  the  method  has  been  to  study  the  simplest  re- 
actions, and  then  take  into  account  the  circumstances  in 
which  our  usual  mental  operations  differ  from  them. 

Conditions  Affecting  Simple  Re-action  Times. 

Referring  to  the  accompanying  tab'e  for  a  general  view 
of  the  time-relations  of  simple  re-actions,  we  may  pass  at  once 
to  the  topic  of  greatest  interest,  viz.,  the  influences  by  which 
they  are  quickened  or  retarded.  These  maybe  considered  as 
(A)  objective,  or  affecting  the  conditions  of  the  experiment, 
and  (B)  subjective,  affecting  the  attitude  of  the  re-actor. 

(A,  1)  The  Nature  of  the  Impression.  The  distinctive 
characteristics  of  a  simple  re-action  being  in  the  attitude  of 
the  re-actor,  it  would  seem  that  its  time  could  be  little  af- 
fected by  the  nature  of  the  impression.  The  motor  signal- 
ling process  is  the  same,  the  connection  between  the  im. 


BIENTAL  PHENOMENA. 


11 


X^ 

— 

HH 
HH 

%K* 

X 
X 

(• 

< 
k- 
K 
H 

j 

^* 

H           "*H 

4            HH 

g^ 

&*Baa  ^ 

M 

M 

*—  t  —  > 

1—  1  t-H  >-< 

? 

-3 

00 

.g   . 

CO 

o 

c 

8 

CO       _i 

-g  J 

g 

(S 

H 

Is- 

v. 

'sL 

n 

B. 

—~A— 

> 

CD         < 

Pi 

&  &  • 

;?    ^ 

0 

!-*» 

B 

o> 

iperaturt 

14 

C^             ^ 

P 

'    *0 

fature  of 

n 

CD 

ff 
| 

0 
M 

o" 

CO 

o  — 

CD_ 

"^  ^4  ^  1—  <  >-H 
•—  I  "^  *<  ^H  ta^          I-H 

^          ? 

CD 
E 

»  -B" 
"o. 

CO 
O> 
B 
CD 

FT 

S 

•d 

f? 

P 

d 

i  s 

B 
Pi 



..... 

1 

B. 

1= 
Pi 

.  . 

^ 

5 

TO 

Be 

B  0 

3 

& 

r* 

r-*   O 

S! 
o 

SB 

•>  — 

'   "TO 

a> 
ji 

B  """** 

'1 

•~- 

O 

& 

m 

B 

0     tt 

^     ftl 

O     W 

S          1 

a  s 

! 

o 

B 
to 

terberg. 
ii 

— 

11 

v_v_, 

0      TO 
P      P 

m 
TO 

0 
0 

i-h 

'  "B 
P 

bserver. 

i 

0 
o>" 

< 

< 

<! 

c 

1 

^ 

^ 

G»  Ul 

VI 

'i  O--  O 
OiS  CO 

to 

O> 

o» 

£ 

C^  -1  -1  CC  4- 

^n  —  .  -1-JOO        i-* 

Ci  CC  O  *>•  4-        O 

^;      CT 

S^g 

1 

1 

8  minutes  after  taking  60  ccra.  of  rum.  — 

gQ  n  .1  41  11  14 

Normal. 

Intensity  of  sound  unknown.  \ 
Attention  directed  to  sensation. 
"  "  movement. 

Intensity  of  sound  known. 

Preparatory  signal  irregularly  varied  with- 
lu  15  seconds. 

Preparatory  signal  at  regular  intervals. 

Ou  the  hand. 
Ou  the  foot. 
With  preparato-y  signal. 
Without  "  " 

The  intensity  in  terms  of  a  common  unit 
were  as  7,  23,  123,  315,  1000,  the  two  high- 
est intensities  not  being  determluable. 

Average  of  all  parts  of  body. 

Remarks. 

GO 

12  THE  TIME-RELATIONS  OF 

pression  and  movement  is  about  equally  artificial  in  all 
cases,  so  that  the  chief  variability  must  be  in  the  time  needed 
for  receiving  the  impression.  For  the  different  senses  this 
time  is  different.  Taking  the  general  average  of  all  the 
observations  accessible  to  me,  I  find,  for  hearing,  1380"; 
for  touch,  148(7;  for  sight,  185(7.  This  order  is  quite  con- 
stant with  the  different  observers,  the  long  time  of  visual 
re-actions  being  referable  to  the  long  inertia  period  of  that 
sense,  as  well  as  to  the  small  perceptive  area  of  the  retina, 
necessitating  a  precise  accommodation,  —  a  condition  not 
always  supplied  in  the  above  experiments.  This  view  is 
strengthened  by  the  shortening  in  the  re-action  time  (by  36(7 
for  Exner,  by  24(7  for  v.  Wittich)  when  the  eye  is  stimu- 
lated electrically.  In  re-acting  to  a  temperature  sensation, 
care  must  be  taken  not  to  re-act  to  the  sensation  of  contact 
with  the  skin.  Where  this  has  been  done  it  has  been  found 
that  the  re-action  to  the  sensations  of  temperature  is  longer 
than  to  contact,  and  I  hat  the  re-action  to  heat  is  longer  than 
to  cold.  Thus,  Vintschgau  and  Steinach  re-act  to  a  pressure 
on  various  points  of  the  head  in  109(7,  to  a  sensation  of  cold 
in  135(7,  of  heat  in  146(7,  similar  values  for  various  points  of 
the  hand  being  121(7,  188(7,  209(7.  The  researches  of  Gold- 
scheider  agree  with  these  in  the  main,  but  make  the  differ- 
ence between  the  re  action  times  to  heat  and  to  cold  much 
greater.  The  senses  of  taste  and  smell  clearly  illustrate  the 
effect  of  the  kind  of  stimulation,  for  here  the  relative  inac- 
cessibility of  the  sense  organs  and  the  slowly  acting  cLemi 
cal  processes  involved  lead  to  a  long  re-action  time.  Though 
experimentation  is  difficult  and  uncertain  in  these  senses,  we 
may  cite  for  smell  the  results  of  Moldenhauer  on  the  odors 
of  various  oils,  centring  about  300(7  (oil  of  roses  273(7,  cam- 
phor 321(7,  musk  319(7,  ether  255(7,  etc.),  and  for  taste,  of 
Honigschmied,  who  re-acts  to  various  tastes  on  the  tip  of  the 
tongue  in  182(7,  though  other  subjects  require  about  300(7. 
On  the  back  of  the  tongue  the  time  is  much  longer,  and  it 


MENTAL   PHENOMENA.  13 

varies  for  different  tastes,  being  longest  for  bitter,  shortest 
for  salt,  and  about  equal  for  sweet  and  sour.  Within  the 
same  sense  the  re-action  time  will  var}'  according  to  the  na- 
ture and  place  of  the  stimulus.  The  above  cited  differences 
for  tastes  and  smells  show  this;  and  for  different  visual  im- 
pressions, for  different  tones,  for  contact  at  different  parts  of 
the  body,  different  results  have  been  obtained,  referable  to 
slight  variations  in  sensibility,  length  of  nerve  traversed, 
clearness  of  the  impression,  and  the  like.  These  minor  dif- 
ferences are  not  easily  established,  but  the  following  may  be 
cited.  Exner  re-acts  to  an  electric  shock  on  the  hand  in 
132(7,  on  the  forehead  in  137(7,  on  the  foot  in  175(7;  v.  Wit- 
tich  re-acts  to  a  point  011  the  back  of  the  finger  in  a  longer 
time  than  to  one  on  the  front,  144(7  and  156(7,  and  regards 
the  difference  as  due  to  a  difference  of  sensibility.  Hall 
and  Kries  clearly  show  that  the  re-action  to  a  point  looked 
at  in  indirect  vision  is  longer  than  to  one  in  direct  vision, 
195(7  and  235(7,  and  find  further  differences  according  as  the 
point  is  above  or  below,  inside  or  outside,  the  retinal  centre. 
A  high  tone  is  re-acted  to  more  quickly  than  a  low  one,  and 
so  on. 

It  is  easier  to  demonstrate  the  influence  of  (2)  the  inten- 
sity of  the  stimulus.  Within  limits,  intense  stimuli  affect 
sense-organs  more  quickly  than  weak  ones,  and,  roughly 
speaking,  an  increase  in  the  intensity  of  the  stimulus  is  con- 
comitant with  a  decrease  in  the  re-action  time.  According 
to  Wuudt,  the  noise  of  a  hammer  falling  respectively  from 
heights  of  1,  4,  8,  and  16  millimetres  was  re-acted  to  in  217(7, 
146(7,  132(7,  and  135(7,  and  the  sound  of  a  ball  falling  from 
heights  of  2,  5,  25,  and  55  centimetres  in  176(7,  161(7,  159(7, 
and  94(7  respectively.  Exner  varied  the  length  and  there- 
fore the  brilliancy  of  an  electric  spark  from  0.5  to  7  milli- 
metres and  obtained  a  steadily  decreasing  re-action  time  of 
158(7  to  123(7.  More  complete  are  the  observations  of  Ber- 
ger  and  Cattell,  who  found  that  as  the  light  increased  from 


14  THE   TIME-RELATIONS  OF 

7  to  23,  to  123,  to  315,  to  1.000,  and  to  two  greater  but  not 
deterrniuable  degrees  of  intensity  (as  compared  with  a  small 
unit  of  light),  the  re-action  *,imes  fell  (average  of  two  ob- 
servers) from  210(5-  to  184(7,  to  174(7,  to  1707,  to  169(7,  to 
156(7,  to  148(7.  For  sound,  as  the  ball  fell  from  heights  of 
60,  160,  300,  and  560  millimetres  the  re-action  times  were 
151  (7,  146(7,  1270",  and  123  ff.  For  electrical  touch  excitations, 
re-actions  to  four  grades  of  stimuli  separated  by  equally 
perceptible  differences  were  made  (average  of  two  observers) 
in  173(7,  159(7,  145(7,  and  145(7.  Wundt  regards  the  differ- 
ence in  re-action  times  of  the  different  senses  as  in  part 

• 

referable  to  differences  in  intensity,  and,  when  re-acting  to 
just  perceptible  intensities  of  sensation  in  various  senses, 
finds  about  the  same  long  time  for  each,  330(7. 

3.  The  Mode  of  Re-action.  The  various  movements  by 
which  we  may  signal  that  a  sensation  has  been  received  may 
differ  in  the  ease  of  their  execution,  in  the  length  of  nerve 
traversed,  as  well  as  in  the  naturalness  of  association  with 
the  impression.  Such  differences,  however,  seem  to  be 
small ;  when  once  the  movement  is  understood  and  antici- 
pated, the  difference  in  the  times  of  its  execution  is  slight. 
Thus,  Miinsterberg  found,  in  testing  the  re-action  of  each  of 
the  five  fingers,  that  while  at  first  the  thumb  and  little  finger 
re-acted  more  slowly  than  the  others,  after  some  practice  the 
times  of  all  were  substantially  the  same.  Fere,  however, 
has  some  results  suggesting  that  the  fingers  making  the 
strongest  movements  re-act  in  the  shortest  times.  Very  in- 
teresting, too,  is  the  experiment  of  Ewald  in  which  the 
stimulus,  an  electric  shock,  is  given  to  the  finger  in  the  very 
key  by  which  the  re-action  is  signalled,  the  re-action  consist- 
ing in  the  very  natural  movement  of  drawing  the  finger 
away.  Under  these  circumstances  he  found  a  brief  and 
constant  time  of  90(7.  Both  Vintschgau  and  Cattell  have 
compared  the  time  of  re-acting  by  closing  a  key  with  the 
finger  and  by  speaking  a  word,  and  find  the  vocal  method 


MENTAL   PHENOMENA.  15 

the  longer  by  about  16 0"  and  30(7  respectively.  Differences 
in  re-acting  from  the  two  sides  of  the  body  have  been  ob- 
served by  some,  the  right  side  showing  the  shorter  re-action, 
but  this  difference  can  hardly  be  considered  as  constant. 
Orchansky  has  shown  in  one  case  that  the  movements  of 
inhibition  take  about  the  same  time  as  those  of  excitation  of 
a  muscle,  and  it  would  be  possible  to  study  the  relative  ease 
of  various  movements  by  this  method.  A  practical  exam  pie 
is  furnished  by  the  commands  of  military  drill,  the  words, 
"carry,"  "present,"  etc.,  announcing  the  mode  of  re-action 
for  the  performance  of  which  the  following  word,  "arms," 
is  the  signal. 

(B)  We  pass  next  to  the  more  important  subjective  fac- 
tors, referring  in  the  main  to  the  expectation  and  the  atten- 
tion. While  nothing  has  been  definitely  said  upon  tins 
point,  the  implication  has  been  that  the  subject  tries  his  ut- 
most to  re-act  as  quickly  as  possible,  and  that  he  knows  the 
nature  of  the  experiment.  While  the  influences  now  to 
be  discussed  seem  to  be  general  in  their  effect,  making  the 
nervous  system  at  one  time  a  better  and  again  a  worse  re- 
acting apparatus,  they  may,  in  certain  respects,  be  subjected 
to  a  more  definite  analysis.  We  begin  with  (1)  the  subject's 
fore-knowledge  of  what  is  to  take  place.  We  may  antici- 
pate the  outcome  of  experimentation  on  this  point  by  for- 
mulating the  law  that  the  more  definite  the  fore-knowledge 
of  the  subject  the  quicker  the  re-action.  Apparently  there 
is  a  process  that  must  be  gone  through  with  in  each  re  action, 
and  the  better  prepared  the  subject  is  for  this, — that  is,  the 
more  of  this  process  gone  through  with  before  the  giving  of 
the  stimulus,  —  the  less  of  it  falls  within  the  measured  inter- 
val. The  precise  nature  of  this  process  is  a  difficult  and 
much  discussed  problem.  It  may  be  sufficient  to  note  at 
present  that  the  re  action  to  a  certain  stimulus  cannot  but 
imply  in  some  sense  the  distinction  of  that  stimulus  from  the 
many  others  by  which  we  are  constantly  surrounded.  If 


16  THE   TIME-RELATIONS  OF 

the  subject  be  re-acting  to  a  visual  impression,  he  will  prob 
ably  not  press  the  key  should  a  noise  occur  in  the  room  or 
something  accidentally  come  in  contact  with  his  hand.  To 
re-act  to  a  visual  impression  thus  implies  the  distinction  of 
that  from  other  impressions.  It  implies  the  identification 
of  the  expected  with  the  existing  impression.  Just  as  we 
recognize  an  appearance  in  the  heavens  or  under  the  micro- 
scope more  readily  when  we  know  where  and  what  to  look 
for,  or  as  we  immediately  recognize  an  almost  forgotten  ac- 
quaintance when  expecting  him,  though  at  a  chance  mealing 
W3  might  have  passed  him  without  recognition,  so  we  re-act 
to  an  impression  most  quickly  when  it  is  most  definitely  ex- 
pected, with  regard  to  its  nature,  its  time,  place  of  ap- 
pearance, and  the  like.  This  expectation  may  be  more  or 
less  specific,  and  an  interesting  series  of  experiments  consists 
in  varying  the  fore-knowledge  of  the  subject  while  still  leav- 
ing it  definite  enough  to  call  the  result  a  simple  re-action, 
(a)  We  may  leave  the  precise  time  of  the  appearance  of  the 
stimulus  undetermined.  This  may  be  done  by  experiment- 
ing with  and  without  a  preparatory  .signal,  preceding  the 
stimulus  by  a  regular  interval.  Wundt  re-acted  to  the  sound 
of  a  ball  falling  from  a  height  of  25  centimetres  in  76(7  with 
a  preparatory  signal,  but  in  253(7  if  no  such  signal  preceded ; 
to  a  ball  falling  five  centimetres,  in  175(7  in  the  first  case,  and 
266(7  in  the  second.  Marti  us  re-acts  to  the  sound  of  a  falling 
hammer  in  127(7  when  preceded  at  a  regular  interval  by  a 
signal,  but  in  178(7  without  the  signal.  Dwelshauvers  re-acts 
in  193(7  in  the  former  case,  in  236(7  in  the  latter  (average  of 
"sensory"  and  "motor"  re-actions).  The  time  between 
the  signal  and  stimulus  is  here  regular,  and  the  most  favora- 
ble time  seems  to  be  about  two  seconds.  Lange  found  the 
time  with  an  interval  of  two  seconds  less  than  with  one  of 
one  or  three  seconds.  Dwelshauvers  varied  the  interval  from  a 
second  and  a  half  to  three,  and  then  to  six  seconds,  and  finds 
the  shortest  re-actions  at  a  second  and  a  half.  Martius  has 


MENTAL    PHENOMENA.  17 

experimented  with  intervals  from  about  one  second  to  one- 
sixth  of  a  second,  and  finds  the  one-second  interval  the  most 
favorable.  If  the  interval  be  irregularly  varied  within  two 
seconds  the  effect  is  hardly  noticeable,  but  if  irregularly 
varied  within  fifteen  seconds  the  time  is  increased  (Cattell). 
With  a  normal  re-action  to  sight  of  1490"  and  to  sound  of 
124(7,  the  re-action  to  sight  with  the  interval  varying  within 
two  seconds  was  148(7;  when  varying  within  fifteen  seconds, 
to  sight  174(7,  to  sound  165(7  (average  of  two  observers). 
Similarly  Martius  finds  that  re-actions  with  an  irregular 
interval  between  signal  and  stimulus  result  in  a  time  inter- 
mediate between  that  with  a  regular  interval  and  that  with- 
out a  signal  at  all.  (6)  If  the  time  and  nature  of  the  stimulus 
be  known,  but  its  intensity  be  varied,  the  time  is  increased. 
When  re-acting  to  a  uniform  change  between  a  feeble  and  a 
loud  sound,  the  re-action  time  to  the  former  was  127(7,  to  the 
latter,  116(7;  but  when  these  changes  were  made  in  an  irregu- 
lar and  unexpected  manner,  the  times  were  lengthened  to 
208(7  and  19S<7. 

In  a  similar  manner  the  attention,  which  we  have  sup- 
posed hitherto  to  be  focused  upon  the  matter  in  hand  with 
a  maximum  of  effort,  may  be  prevented  from  being  ef- 
fectually directed  to  the  making  of  the  re-action  by  a  variety 
of  circumstances.  Some  of  these  we  may  group  under  the 
term  (2)  distraction.  By  a  constant  noise  or  other  means 
we  may  be  creating  a  stimulus  to  which  the  attention  is  in- 
voluntarily drawn,  and  thus  withdrawn  from  the  process  of 
re-action.  Wundt  re-acted  to  a  sound  of  mean  intensity  in 
189(7,  to  a  strong  sound  in  158(7,  but  when  a  disturbing 
sound  was  going  on  in  the  room  these  re-actions  required 
313(7  and  203(7.  On  the  other  hand,  with  Cattell,  when  in 
good  practice,  so  that  the  re-action  became  almost  automatic, 
the  effect  of  a  disturbing  sound  both  upon  sight  and  sound 
re-actions  was  insignificant, — normal  for  sight  149(7,  with 
disturbing  noise  155(7;  normal  for  sound  124(7,  with  dis- 


18  THE  TIME-RELATIONS   OP 

turbing  noise  124(7.  Dwelshauvers  finds  a  longer  re-action 
time  when  a  minimum  of  attention  is  paid  to  the  re-action. 
It  is  quite  probable  that  what  acts  as  a  disturbance  to  one 
person  hardly  affects  another.  In  some  individuals  the  re- 
action time  seems  to  be  extremely  sensitive  to  any  mental 
disturbance.  One  of  Obersteiner's  subjects,  with  an  average 
re-action  time  of  about  100(7,  requires  142(7  to  re-act  when 
music  is  heard,  and  another's  re  action  time  is  lengthened  by 
100(7  when  talking  is  going  on  in  the  room. 

A  more  general  and  thorough  form  of  distraction  may  be 
effected  by  imposing  a  task  requiring  distinct  mental  effort 
at  the  same  time  that  the  re-action  is  to  take  place.  Thus 
Cattell  attempted  to  add  17  consecutively  to  a  series  of  num- 
bers, and  found  that  re  actions  taken  while  this  was  going 
on  were  longer  by  28(7  (average  of  two  observers).  All  such 
effects  seem  to  be  much  more  marked  when  the  re-action  in 
question  is  new  than  when  it  has  become  familiar  and  partly 
automatic.  The  disturbance  seems  to  act  by  delaying  the 
association  between  stimulus  and  movement. 

(3)  We  have  now  to  notice  a  distinction  which,  though 
but  recently  brought  to  light  (by  N.  Lange,  1888),  is  of  fun- 
damental importance.  A  re-action  may  be  made  in  two 
ways.  In  the  one  form  of  re-action  the  attention  is  directed 
to  the  expected  impression:  it  is  identified  as  the  expected 
impression,  and  thereupon  is  initiated  the  impulse  resulting 
in  the  re- acting  movement.  The  several  processes  are  per- 
formed serially,  the  attention  being  concentrated  upon  the 
sensory  part  of  the  process.  In  the  other  form  of  re-action 
the  attention  is  directed  to  the  movement:  the  impulse  is 
ready,  and  is  set  off  by  the  appearance  of  the  signal  almost 
automatically,  the  identification  of  the  actual  with  the  ex- 
pected impression  being  omitted.  The  first  is  spoken  of  as 
the  "complete"  or  "sensory"  mode  of  re-action ;  the  second, 
as  the  "  shoi'tened  "or  "  motor  "  form.  In  the  experiments 
of  Lange  the  simple  sensory  re-action  time  to  a  sound  (aver- 


MENTAL  PHENOMENA.  Ill 

age  of  three  persons)  was  227(7,  motor  123(7;  to  a  visual  im- 
pression (average  of  two  persons),  sensory  290(7,  motor  113d; 
to  a  tactile  impression  (one  person),  sensory  213(7,  motor 
108(7. 

These  differences,  however,  seem  to  be  rather  extreme. 
Miinsterberg  finds  for  sound,  sensory  162(7,  motor  120(7. 
Martius  finds  (average  of  various  experiments)  for  a  "sen- 
sory "  re  action  to  a  sound  161(7,  for  a  "  motor"  141(7,  but  in 
two  subjects  not  practised  in  re-acting  210<T  and  111(7. 
Dwelshauvers  finds  a  difference  as  great  as  between  279(7  and 
137(7  for  three  subjects.  It  should  be  noted  that  this  differ- 
ence in  the  attitude  of  the  subject  is  not  always  easy  to 
maintain,  and  that  it  is  at  times  equally  difficult  to  decide 
whether  a  re-action  has  been  "sensory"  or  "  motor."  Un- 
doubtedly transitional  modes  of  re-action  occur  in  which  the 
attention  is  not  sharply  focused  on  either  stimulus  or  move- 
ment, but  rather  divided  between  the  two;  and  it  maybe 
that  this  is  the  most  usual  mode  of  re-action  when  the  distinc- 
tion is  not  taken  in  to  account.  Both  Martius  and  Dwelshauvers 
have  recorded  the  subject's  own  opinion  of  the  quickness  of 
each  re-action,  and  the  state  of  his  attention  at  the  moment 
of  re-acting.  The  estimate  of  the  relative  speed  of  re-action 
seems  to  be  fairly  accurate,  and  those  re  actions  seem  to  be 
shortest  in  which  the  attention  of  the  subject  was  sharply 
focused  upon  the  matter  in  hand.  A  further  characteristic 
of  the  motor  form  of  re-action  is  that  its  average  varia- 
tion is  smaller,  i.e.,  the  process  is  more  regular;  and  that 
false  re-actions  occur,  either  anticipations  of  stimulus  or 
re-actions  to  some  accidental  disturbance.  The  distinc- 
tion is  important  as  aiding  in  the  explanation  of  individ- 
ual differences,  as  well  as  of  the  path  of  practice.  The 
somewhat  conflicting  results  obtained  before  this  distinc- 
tion was  taken  into  account  might  very  well  be  due  to 
the  fact  that  the  one  observer  re-acted  in  the  one  way, 
and  the  other  in  the  other.  Thus  the  re-action  times  of 


20  THE  TIME-RELATIONS  OF 

Kries  and  Auerbach  are  motor;  for  they  are  brief,  false  re- 
actions occur,  and  it  is  noted  that  the  simple  re-actions  fol- 
lowing re-actions  involving  distinctions  were  longer  by  410" 
and  Biff  than  before,— a  change  probably  due  to  a  return  to 
a  partially  sensory  mode  of  re-action.  Again,  there  are 
doubtless  transitional  modes  between  the  two,  and  there  are 
reasons  for  believing  that  the  path  of  practice  is  from  the 
sensory  to  the  motor  form  of  re-action. 

The  influences  that  remain  to  be  discussed  may  be  consid- 
ered under  the  heads  of  "practice,"  "fatigue,"  'individual 
differences,"  and  "abnormal  variations."  (4)  Practice.  As 
just  noticed,  the  effect  of  practice  is  intimately  connected 
with  the  mode  of  re-action.  It  is  noticed  by  almost  all 
writers,  but  the  extent  to  which  it  influences  the  time  is  very 
various.  The  observations  make  it  probable  that  the  effect 
of  practice  is  most  marked  at  first,  and  that  when  once  the 
initial  stages  are  over,  the  effect  of  continued  practice  is  small. 
It  is  greatest  in  those  persons  whose  time  is  longest  at  first, 
and  seems  most  influential  in  acts  that  are  complicated  and 
lie  somewhat  beyond  the  realm  of  daily  experience. 

When  the  action  is  once  thoroughly  learned,  an  interval 
of  disuse  seems  not  to  affect  the  time  seriously.  After  not 
re-acting  for  three  months,  Cattell  found  no  essential  differ- 
ence in  the  time.  On  the  other  hand,  with  some  there  is  a 
slight  newness  on  beginning  each  day's  work,  making  the 
first  re-actions  of  a  series  rather  long  (Trautschoklt). 

(5)  A  similar  statement  may  be  made  of  fatigue:  it  has 
greatest  effect  upon  the  complicated,  less  thoroughly  learned 
processes,  and  varies  with  the  individual  and  the  mode  of 
re  action.  With  an  automatic  simple  process  its  effect  is 
very  slow  to  appear  (Cattell.  It  may  enter  at  any  stage  of 
the  process,  sensory,  motor,  or  central ;  but  the  last  seems  to 
be  the  most  serious.  It  appears  as  a  difficulty  in  keeping 
one's  attention  upon  the  experiment,  and  thus  lengthens  the 
time,  and  especially  the  average  variation  of  the  experi- 


MENTAL   PHENOMENA.  21 

mrnts.  By  fatigue  is  meant  the  fatigue  brought  about  by 
the  experimenting  itself.  The  time  is  also  affected  by  gen- 
eral fatigue  preceding  tbe  experiment.  Some  individuals 
are  extremely  sensitive  to  influences  of  this  kind. 

(6)  Individual  Variations.  The  fact  here  to  be  investi- 
gated is  the  general  one  that  different  persons  require  differ- 
ent times  for  the  performance  of  the  same  operations.  The 
difficulty  of  drilling  a  company  of  men  to  act  in  concert, 
whether  in  military  drill  or  otherwise,  springs  in  part  from 
this  difference.  It  was  from  this  point  of  view,  too,  that 
the  time  of  mental  processes  was  first  studied.  So  long  ago 
as  1795  Maskelyne,  the  astronomer  i-oyal,  discharged  his  as- 
sistant because  the  latter  recorded  the  transit  of  a  star  across 
the  wire  of  the  telescope  half  a  second  or  more  later  than  he 
himself.  Some  twenty-five  years  later  Bessel,  another  as- 
tronomer, had  his  attention  called  to  the  point,  and  upon  in- 
vestigation established  the  fact  that  no  two  observers  re- 
corded such  ti-ansits  at  precisely  the  same  time.  The  differ- 
ence in  time  between  any  two  observers  was  usually 
expressed  as  an  equation,  and  hence  the  term  "personal 
equation,"  which,  though  strictly  applicable  only  to  the 
differences  so  found,  has  assumed  a  much  wider  meaning. 
The  individual  differences  become  greater  as  the  process  to 
be  performed  increases  in  complexity,  and  this  explains  in 
part  why  the  personal  equations  as  determined  by  the  com- 
plicated eye  and  ear  method  were  so  large:  with  the  simpler 
method  of  electrical  record  these  d  ifferences  are  much  reduced . 
Besides  the  differences  due  to  practice  and  the  mode  of  re- 
action, there  are  a  large  number  of  minor  sources  of  varia- 
tion, which  as  yet  are  not  sufficiently  understood  to  justify 
a  correlation  of  quick  or  slow  re-action  times  with  definite 
individual  qualities.  We  may,  however,  note  (a)  that  the 
time  is  longer  in  children  than  in  adults,  as  has  been  shown, 
amongst  others,  by  Binet,  who  found  that  children  from  3^ 
to  7  years  re-acted  in  from  440(7  to  6600",  when  adults  required 


22  THE  TIME-RELATIONS  OP 

but  140(7.  In  the  very  old  the  time  is  longer  than  in  the 
prime  of  life.  Under  the  influence  of  mental  or  physical 
fatigue,  worry,  or  slight  indisposition,  the  time  has  been  in- 
creased. Obersteiner,  Vintschgau,  Goldscheider,  and  others, 
have  incidentally  observed  these  effects,  showing  an  increase 
of  30i  to  40(7.  These  variations  are  related  to  others,  shad- 
ing over  into  the  abnormal.  Under  this  head  may  be  con- 
sidered (7)  the  action  of  drugs  and  re-action  times  in  the 
insane.  Several  of  the  earlier  experimenters  made  a  few 
observations  concerning  the  effect  of  drugs.  Exner  found 
quite  a  marked  lengthening  of  the  time  after  drinking  wine. 
Vintschgau  and  Dietl  found  that  the  effect  of  coffee  was  to 
decrease  and  of  morphium  to  increase  the  time  for  a  consid- 
erable period.  The  more  elaborate  researches  of  Kraepelin 
show  that  the  effect  of  amyl,  ether,  and  chloroform  is  a 
sudden  lengthening  of  the  re-action  times,  reaching  a  maxi- 
mum in  a  very  few  minutes,  and  followed  by  a  rather  long 
period  of  times  slightly  shorter  than  the  normal.  If  a  strong 
dose  of  the  drug  be  used  the  lengthening  is  more  considera- 
ble and  the  secondary  shortening  slighter.  Thus  Kraepelin, 
whose  normal  re-action  was  183(7,  after  a  strong  inhalation 
of  ether  re-acted  in  298(7,  and  in  the  period  of  shortening  in 
170(7;  while  with  a  light  narcosis  the  maximum  re-action 
was  223(7,  and  the  shortened  re-action  150(7.  The  effect  of 
alcohol,  however,  is  a  brief  period  of  shortened  times  fol- 
lowed by  a  long  period  of  lengthened  times.  This  is  also 
found  by  Orchansky,  who,  with  a  normal  re-action  of  155(7, 
re-acts  in  105(7  eight  minutes  after  taking  a  dose  of  alcohol, 
and  in  225(7  after  thirty  minutes.  The  observations  of  War- 
ren do  not  yield  equally  positive  results,  but  do  not  conflict 
with  those  of  Kraepelin.  Changes  in  the  extent  of  the  aver- 
age variation  have  also  been  observed.  On  what  psycho- 
logical factors  these  differences  depend  it  is  difficult  to  say, 
but  the  subjective  feelings  accompanying  the  lengthened 
times  are  a  difficulty  in  keeping  the  attention  upon  the  mat- 


MENTAL   PHENOMENA. 

ter  iu  hand,  and  an  unwillingness  to  exert  one's  self.  The 
evidence  afforded  by  the  action  of  drugs  upon  these  pro- 
cesses is  important  as  indicating  the  dependence  of  the 
re-actions  upon  physiological  conditions.  A  change  of  re- 
action times  in  insanity  has  been  frequently  observed,  but 
the  field  for  individual  variation  is  here  very  large.  It 
seems  probable  that  in  most  forms  of  mental  disease,  and 
particularly  in  melancholia,  there  is  a  considerable  length- 
ening of  the  re-action  time,  amounting  in  extreme  cases  to 
one  half  or  three-quarters  of  a  second.  In  the  excited  forms 
of  disease,  such  as  mania,  a  shortening  has  been  observed. 
Obersteiner  cites  a  case  of  general  paralysis  in  the  incipient 
stages  of  which  the  thne  was  1660",  in  a  more  advanced 
stage  2810",  in  a  most  advanced  stage  451<T.  Stanley  Hall 
has  found  a  marked  shortening  of  the  time  iu  the  hypnotic 
condition,  but  his  result  is  not  corroborated  by  others. 

Methods  of  Experimentation. 

The  chief  requisite  in  these  experiments  is  an  apparatus 
for  accurately  measuring  small  intervals  of  time.  The  ear- 
liest method,  still  in  use,  records  the  vibrations  of  a  tuning- 
fork  upon  the  quickly-moving  smoked  surface  of  a  rotating 
drum,  and  beneath  this  the  moment  of  giving  the  signal  and 
making  the  response.  If  a  fork  making  one  hundred  vibra- 
tions per  second  be  used,  whole  hundredths  can  be  directly 
counted  and  smaller  fractions  estimated.  Wundt  has  con- 
structed a  more  accurate  and  specialized  instrument  in  which 
a  fork  making  five  hundred  vibrations  per  second  is  used. 
A  very  much  simplified  form  of  apparatus  has  been  devised 
by  Obersteiner,  in  which  the  slide  holding  the  record  is 
moved  by  hand,  and  the  movement  of  re-action  draws  the 
fork  off  the  record;  and  by  Bowditch,  in  which  the  fork  it- 
self carries  the  record,  and  the  signal  and  re-action  are  indi- 
cated by  a  shifting  of  the  writing  point.  In  the  astronomical 
records  clock-work  takes  the  place  of  a  tuning  fork.  The 
objection  to  these  methods  is  that  they  necessitate  te.lious 
counting  of  curves.  If  the  rate  of  the  rotating-apparalus  is 
very  uniform  and  frequently  tested,  one  may  substitute 


24  THE   TIME-RELATIONS   OF 

measuring-  for  counting-,  but  the  most  convenient  apparatus 
for  the  purpose  is  the  Hipp  chronoscope.     This  instrument 
contains  a  fine  clock-work,   set  iu   motion  by  releasing  a 
spring  and  running  for  about  half  a  minute.     The  hands  of 
the  two  dials,  the  one  indicating  tenths  and  the  other  thou- 
sandths of  a  second,  do  not  move  until  drawn  away  from  a 
set  of  cogs  by  the  opening  or  closure  of  a  magnetic  circuit, 
and  are  stopped  again   in  the  same  way.     By  making-  the 
usual  arrangement  whereby  the  production  of  the  stimulus 
sets  the  hands  in  motion  and  the  re-acting  movement  brings 
them  to  a  stand-still,  we  can  read  off  directly  the  interval  of 
the  re-action  time.     Unless  we  can  afford  to  sacrifice  accu- 
racy for  convenience,  a  means  of  controlling   the  chrono- 
scope is  indispensable.     This  may  be  done  by  timing  the  fall 
of  a  ball  from  a  given  height  and  comparing  it  with  the  theo- 
retical time.     In  the  apparatus  for  this    purpose  supplied 
with  the  chronoscope  the  ball  is  mechanically  released,  and 
the  mode  of  making  the  circuit  is  equally  defective,  so  that 
the  error  of  the  control  apparatus  is  probably  greater  than 
that  of  the  chronoscope.     To  obviate  this  difficulty  I   make 
use  of  a  ball  held  in  position  by  a  magnet,  and  failing  from 
any  height  up  to  seven  feet,  upon  the  arm  of  a  well-balanced 
lever,  thus  securing  an  instantaneous  release.     By  setting  the 
magnet  and  ball  at  different  distances  we  are  also  enabled  to 
decide  whether  the  error  is  absolute  or  relative.     It  is  here 
necessary  to  break  the  current  by  which  the  ball  falls,  and 
to  make  the  current  by  which  the  chronoscope  starts  at  the 
same  moment;  this  is  effected  by  a  key  specially  devised  for 
the  purpose.     The  chronoscope   possesses   a  regulation   for 
altering   its   rate    when  too  slow  or  too  fast,  but  I  find  it 
most  convenient  to  make  sparing  use  of   this,  and  apply  a 
correction  for  each  day's  determinations  as  found  with  the 
•'fall  apparatus."     Another  form  of  control  makes  use  of  a 
falling  hammer,  the  record  being  also  made  with  a  tuning- 
fork.     A  recent  device  of  Ewald  combines  the  two  methods 
by  mechanically  counting  the  vibrations  of  a  tuning-fork:  a 
delicate  armature  is  drawn  to  and  released  by  a  magnet  with 
each  vibration  of  the  fork,  moving  the  hand  of  a  dial  over 
one  of  its  divisions  as  it  does  so.     The  fork  is  vibrating  con- 
stantly, but  the  making  of  the  signal  sends  the  current  into 
the   u  interruption-counter,"  while  the  re-acting  movement 
again  diverts  the  current  away  from  it.     It  will  record  at 


IUKNTAL   PHEXOMFXA.  25 

the  rate  of  one  hundred  per  second.  Galton  has  constructed 
for  ordinary  uses  a  machine  in  which  the  signal  is  given  by 
the  release  of  a  rod  or  pendulum,  and  the  re-acting  move- 
ment mechanically  arrests  the  fall  or  swing,  a  scale  of  in- 
terval being  determined  for  the  apparatus  empirically.  San- 
ford  has  devised  a  simple  but  not  expeditious  chronoscope, 
in  which  the  signal  and  the  response  separately  set  in  mo- 
tion two  pendulums  of  slightly  different  periods,  the  re-act- 
ing interval  being  calculated  from  the  number  of  oscillations 
occurring  before  the  two  are  in  unison. 

The  methods  of  indicating  the  moment  at  which  the 
signal  appeared  and  the  moment  at  which  the  re-acting 
movement  was  made  are  simple.  When  the  record  is  writ- 
ten on  a  rotating  surface,  a  point  connected  with  a  magnet, 
and  writing  a  straight  line  beneath  the  vibrations  of  the 
fork,  writes  that  line  at  a  different  level  when  the  signal  is 
given,  and  returns  to  the  same  level  when  the  re-acting 
movement  is  made;  or  the  tuning-fork  itself  may  be  made 
to  write  at  a  different  level  during  the  interval  measured. 
The  arrangement  by  which  the  level  is  changed  on  the  rec- 
ord, or  the  hands  are  set  in  motion  in  the  chronoscope  at 
the  same  instant  that  the  stimulus  appears,  is  equally  sim- 
ple. For  sound,  the  noise  of  the  key  by  which  the  circuit 
is  made  is  generally  sufficient,  or  other  sound  may  be  pro- 
duced by  bodies  falling  upon  various  surfaces  and  thereby 
opening  or  closing  a  key.  For  sight,  the  impression  to 
which  a  re-action  is  to  bo  made  may  be  concealed  behind  a 
screen,  and  the  drawing  away  of  this  screen  at  the  same 
time  makes  or  breaks  an  electric  circuit.  Frequently  the 
re-actor  sits  in  the  dark,  and  the  impression  becomes  visible 
only  when  an  electric  spark  appears,  or  the  spark  itself  may 
be  the  stimulus.  For  touch,  temperature,  and  taste,  a  typi- 
cal device  is  that  of  Vintschgau,  in  which  the  end  of  a  rod 
touches  the  sensitive  surface,  and  the  pressure  so  exerted 
makes  a  contact  with  a  delicate  metallic  blade  inserted  in 
the  same  apparatus.  For  smell,  the  movement  by  which 
the  odor  is  set  free  is  similarly  utilized.  The  re-acting 
movement  is  usually  that  of  pressing  an  ordinary  telegraphic 
key.  Devices  have  been  constructed  by  which  movements 
of  the  foot,  of  the  jaw,  of  the  voice  and  lip,  may  be  similarly 
noted.  For  more  detailed  descriptions  consult  the  references 
under  this  head  at  the  end. 


»b  THE   TIME-RELATIONS   OF 

Having-  thus  considered  the  time-relations  of  a  simple  re- 
action, we  may  proceed,  on  the  line  of  analysis  there  laid 
down,  to  the  consideration  of  the  more  complex  forms  of 
re-action. 

Adaptive  Re-actions. 

It  has  been  noted  that  the  prominent  characteristic 
of  a  useful  re-action  is  the  adaptation  of  the  response 
to  the  excitation  by  which  it  was  called  out.  This 
adaptation  involves  a  recognition  of  the  stimulus,  and 
its  association  with  the  movement  in  question.  In  this 
recognition  we  found  it  convenient  to  distinguish  between 
the  recognition  of  the  presence  and  that  of  the  nature  of  the 
stimulus ;  but  it  may  be  questioned  whether  we  can  recognize 
the  presence  except  by  noting  some  point  of  the  nature  of 
the  stimulus,  and  whether  the  noting  of  this  point  does  not 
involve  its  distinction  from  others.  If,  in  re-acting  to  a 
sound,  I  recognize  that  it  is  the  stimulus  to  which  I  am  to 
re-act,  and  press  the  key,  does  this  mean  that  I  know  that 
the  stimulus  is  not  a  visual  or  a  tactile  one,  that  it  is  not  a 
higher  or  a  lower,  a  louder  or  a  feebler,  sound  ?  Here,  as 
still  more  in  the  analysis  to  follow,  our  experimental  basis 
is  defective. 

Experiment  has  naturally  followed  the  lines  of  conven- 
ience and  ready  analysis;  and  as  there  has  been  little  har- 
mony in  these  analyses,  and  as  the  one  here  adopted  differs 
somewhat  from  those  adopted  by  other  writers,  it  will  be 
difficult  to  maintain  the  parallelism  between  theoretical 
discussion  and  the  obtained  results.  If  we  understand  by 
the  simple  re-action  the  mere  signalling  that  a  definite,  pre 
designated,  and  expected  stimulus  is  present,  and  by  an 
"adaptive"  re-action  one  in  which  the  mode  of  response 
depends  upon  and  varies  with  the  nature  of  the  stimulus,  we 
may  distinguish  several  stages  of  connection  between  the 
two.  as  shown  in  the  schedule  at  the  top  of  the  following 
page. 


MENTAL   PHENOMENA.  27 


I.  A  single  stimulus  with  a  single  mode  of  re-action. 
II.  Several  stimuli  with  a  single  mode  of  re-act  inn. 
(a)  The  subject  foreknows  the  stimulus. 
(6)  The  subject  does  not  foreknow  the  stimulus. 
III.  A  single  stimulus  icith  several  modes  of  re-action. 


SIMPI.K  UK- 


IV.  Several  stimuli  with  several  modes  of  re-action.  ACTIONS. 

(a)  The  subject  foreknows  the  stimulus  and  al  o    he 
re-action. 

(b)  The  subject  foreknows  the  re- action,  but  not  the 
stimulus 

(O  The  subject  foreknows  neither  stimulus  )    ADAPTIVE  RE.ACTION. 
nor  re-action.  ( 

Or,  more  simply,  if  the  re-action  is  foreknown,  the  pro- 
cess is  a  simple  re-action  ;  if  not,  it  is  an  adaptive  re-action. 
In  addition,  in  the  simple  re-action  the  foreknowledge  of  the 
stimulus  may  be  entirely  definite,  the  stimulus  always  being 
the  same,  or  there  may  be  a  known  range  of  variation  or  an 
unknown  range  of  variation ;  while  in  the  adaptive  re-action 
the  possibilities  are  limited  to  the  latter  t\vo. 

I.  has  been  fully  considered.  In  IT.  (a)  we  have  a  num- 
ber of  different  simple  re-actions;  but,  instead  of  investigat- 
ing them  in  separate  series,  we  have  different  kinds  in  one 
series:  e.g..  a  sound,  a  light,  or  a  touch  may  appear,  it  being 
announced  to  the  subject  which  it  is  to  be;  and  he  in  each 
case  re-acts  by  pressing  the  key.  The  impressions  may  be 
more  homogeneous,  as  a  series  of  colors;  but  in  all  cases  the 
subject  need  not  appreciate  the  nature  of  the  stimulus,  but 
simply  that  a  stimulus  has  appeared.  In  II.  (6)  the  subject 
knows  the  possible  stimuli,  but  does  not  know  which  is  to 
come  next;  otherwise  the  conditions  are  precisely  the  same 
as  above.  Wuiidt's  experiment  with  the  irregular  change 
between  two  intensities  of  sound  would  belong  here,  and 
would  indicate  that  this  is  an  essential  factor.  In  III.  the 
several  modes  of  re-action  are  necessarily  known  in  advance. 
Instead  of  testing  the  different  forms  of  re-actions  in  separate 
series,  we  have  several  in  one  series.  For  example :  we  re. 
act  to  a  sound  now  with  the  thumb,  then  with  the  I'oivlinger, 
the  subject  always  knowing  in  advance  what  he  is  to  do.  In 


28  THE   TIME-RELATIONS   OF 

IV.  (a)  we  are  combining  into  one  series  different  forms  of 
simple  re-actions,  differing  both  in  stimulus  and  form  of  re- 
action: but  the  complete  reaction  (e.g.,  red  color  to  be 
re-acted  to  by  middle  finger)  is  announced  beforehand.  In 
IV.  (6)  the  subject  is  told  in  advance  how  to  re-act,  but  not 
what  the  stimulus  is  to  be.  However,  in  both  this  and  the 
foregoing  case  he  need  not  wait  to  recognize  the  nature  of 
the  stimulus,  but  re  acts  as  soon  as  he  detects  its  presence. 
All  these  are  variations  of  simple  re-action  times.  When  we 
pass  to  IV.  (c),  we  have  a  different,  namely,  an  adaptive, 
re-action.  The  subject  is  not  told  any  thing  in  advance  ex- 
cept the  association  upon  which  he  is  to  re- act:  e.g.,  if  a  blue 
light,  with  the  right  hand ;  if  a  red  light,  with  the  left  hand  ; 
and  so  on.  The  essential  difference  here  is  that  the  subject 
must  first  distinguish  a  certain  feature  of  the  nature  of  the 
stimulus,  in  this  case  the  color;  then  call  up  the  appropriate 
movement  and  perform  it.  A  re-action  of  this  kind,  there- 
fore, involves  a  definite  distinction  of  stimuli,  and  a  choice  of 

movements. 

Distinction  and  Choice. 

The  mental  processes  involved  in  an  adaptive  re-action, 
in  addition  to  those  involved  in  the  simple  re  action, 
are  thus  a  more  specific  recognition  of  the  stimulus, 
and  a  choice  between  movements.  By  maintaining  all 
other  factors  alike,  the  difference  of  time  of  the  two 
modes  of  re-action  measures  the  combined  time  of  distinction 
and  choice.  The  first  determinations  of  this  nature  were 
made  by  Donders  and  his  pupils  (1865-68^.  A  simple  re- 
action to  a  light,  white  or  red,  was  made  in  201(7  (average 
of  five  observers) ;  but  an  adaptive  re-action  with  the  right 
hand  for  the  one  light,  and  the  left  hand  for  the  other,  in 
355o-,_a  difference  of  154(7.  Cattell  makes  a  simple  adap- 
tive re-action  to  two  colors  in  340(7,  his  simple  re-action  time 
being  146(7,  or  a  difference  of  194(7  (XL).  Miinsterberg  re- 
acts simply  with  any  of  the  five  fingers  in  141(7,  but  re-acts 


MENTAL   1'HKNOMEXA.  2!t 

willi  M  definite  finger  (according  as  the  numbers  of  the  fin- 
gers "one,"  "two,"'  "three,"  etc.,  are  called)  in  195(7  longer 
(XXIV.).  Accepting  these  as  values  for  the  combined  dis- 
tinction and  choice  time  under  simple  conditions,  our  next 
step  would  naturally  be  to  determine  how  much  of  the  time 
is  due  to  distinction,  how  much  to  choice.  This  is  a  diffi- 
cult step;  for  we  cannot  readily  determine  that  a  distinction 
has  been  made,  except  by  indicating  it  in  the  mode  of  re- 
action, and  we  cannot  execute  a  choice  except  upon  the  basis 
of  some  distinction.  The  most  usual  experiment  by  which 
it  has  been  attempted  to  overcome  this  difficulty  consists  in 
re-acting  to  only  a  designated  one  of  a  group  of  stimuli,  allow- 
ing all  others  to  pass  without  re-action.  To  take  a  simple 
case,  let  red  and  blue  be  the  possible  stimuli:  if  red  appears, 
re-act;  if  blue,  do  nothing.  While  this  form  of  experiment 
is  interesting  and  useful,  the  inferences  from  it  are  not  as 
clear  as  could  be  wished.  It  may  be  termed  the  "incomplete 
adaptive  re-action,"  or  briefly  the  "incomplete  re-action." 
It  involves  a  distinction  of  the  stimulus  to  be  re  acted  to, 
from  those  not  to  be  re-acted  to,  and  a  choice  between 
motion  and  refraining  from  action.  It  seems  probable  that 
these  processes  are  respectively  easier  than  a  distinction  that 
cannot  be  anticipated  and  a  choice  between  two  movements; 
but  it  seems  equally  probable  that  the  extent  of  these  differ- 
ences will  vary  considerably  under  different  circumstances. 
If  the  simple  re- action  is  of  the  quick,  motor  form,  and  the 
incomplete  re-action  involves  an  additional  distinction  of 
the  stimulus,  as  well  as  the  choice  between  motion  and  rest, 
the  additional  time  above  the  simple  re-action  would  be  long, 
and  the  difference  between  it  and  the  adaptive  re-action 
short.  This  is  evidently  the  case  with  Cattell  and  Berger, 
who,  with  a  simple  re  action  of  146  ff  and  150(7,  perform  the 
incomplete  re-action  in  306(7  and  277(7,  the  adaptive  in  340(7 
and  295(7  (IV.  and  XL).  On  the  other  hand,  Dondrrs,  with 
an  evidently  sensory  mode  of  re-action,  has  a  simple  re- 


30  THE   TIME-RELATIONS   OF 

action  of  201(7,  an  incomplete  of  237 (7,  and  an  adaptive  of 
284(7.  A  second  method  attempts  to  deal  with  the  difficulty 
by  delaying  the  re-action  until  the  precise  nature  of  the 
stimulus  has  been  appreciated,  and  regards  the  difference  in 
time  between  this  and  the  simple  re  action  as  '  he  time  needed 
for  the  distinction  of  the  stimulus.  There  is  nothing  but  the 
subjective  guaranty  that  the  moment  of  re-action  is  coinci- 
dent with  the  process  of  recognition,  and  we  have  no  reason 
to  regard  this  guaranty  as  valid.  There  may  be  a  tendency 
to  make  the  distinction  on  the  basis  of  the  after-image,  and 
thus  signal  the  appreciation  of  it  too  soon;  or,  again,  an 
extreme  desire  not  to  re-act  before  the  distinction  is  made 
may  delay  reaction  to  an  unusual  length.  Friederich's 
investigations  show  for  colors  a  simple  re-action  time  of  175(T, 
and  a  "subjective  distinction"  time  of  267(7  (XXXIX.); 
while  Tigerstedt,  and  also  Tischer,  find  only  about  half  this 
difference  for  nearly  the  same  re-action..  It  seems  wisest, 
under  these  circumstances,  not  to  decide  the  relative  shares  of 
the  distinction  and  choice  in  the  adaptive  re  action,  but  to 
study  the  combined  time  as  a  whole,  and  the  influences  by 
which  it  is  affected.  We  can  thus  utilize  the  results  of  all 
the  methods  for  comparative  purposes;  and,  in  addition,  we 
can  vary  the  complexity  of  the  distinction  while  leaving  the 
choice  the  same  (and  to  a  more  limited  extent  can  vary  the 
choice  without  the  distinction),  and  thus  can  in  many  cases 
distinguish  whether  an  increased  complexity  of  an  adaptive 
re  action  is  to  be  referred  to  an  increase  in  the  difficulty  of 
the  choice  or  to  an  increase  in  the  difficulty  of  the  distinc- 
tion. 

It  is  desirable  to  analyze  more  particularly  the  nature  of 
the  difference  between  the  simple  re-action  and  the  "sub- 
jective," and  between  the  simple  and  the  incomplete.  An 
essential  point  relates  to  the  mode  of  re  action,  whether  motor 
or  sensory ;  nor  is  it  necessary  that  the  same  mode  of  re-action 
be  followed  in  all  cases.  A  few  points  here  relevant  are 
illustrated  in  Tischer's  results  upon  nine  subjects  with  sound 


MKNTAL    PHENOMENA  31 

re-actions  and  distinctions.  The  average  of  the  nine  gives  a 
distinction  time  of  159(T,  and  a  simple  time  of  I18o".  Four 
of  the  subjects  evidently  make  vise  of  the  motor  re-action, 
their  simple  time  being  107(7,  and  their  subjective  distinction 
116(7;  i.e.,  the  re-action  takes  place  upon  the  appearance  of  the 
stimulus,  its  distinction  taking  place  later.  Berger  and  Cat- 
tell  express  the  same  difficulty,  and  for  this  reason  discarded 
the  method.  Their  simple  re-action  to  weak  liyht  was  198(7, 
with  distinction  of  intensity  208(7.  Two  of  the  subjects 
evidently  7-e-act  according  to  the  "sensory"  method,  their  sim- 
ple re-action  being  141(7,  and  the  subjective  distinction  246(7. 
That  these  are  not  individual  differences  is  shown  by  the 
fact  that  the  adaptive  re-actions  are  about  alike  in  all. 
Similarly  with  regard  to  the  difference  between  the  simple 
and  the  incomplete  re  action  times.  Dondeis,  and  those  of 
Tischer's  subjects  who  re-act  by  the  "sensory"  method,  show  a 
relatively  small  difference,  though  this  is  not  true  of  Friede- 
rich's  subjects.  While  Tischer's  "motor"  subjects  show  a 
difference  of  159(7  between  the  simple  and  the  incomplete, 
the  "  sensory  "  subjects  show  one  of  only  61(7.  Again,  when 
the  re-action  is  "  motor,"  the  expectation  is  entirely  directed 
to  the  stimulus  upon  which  re-action  is  to  follow,  and  the 
fact  that  other  stimuli  may  appear  hardly  enters  into  the 
experiment.  This  seems  to  he  the  case  with  Kries  and  Auer- 
bach,  who,  with  clearly  "motor"  re-actions,  find  a  difference 
of  30-40(7  for  (XXX.-XXXYJ.)  a  variety  of  incomplete  re- 
actions. It  may  be  added  that  the  change  from  the  simple  to 
the  incomplete  form  of  re-action  will  often  bring  with  it  an  at- 
tention to  the  sensory  part  of  the  process,  and  thus  make  the 
difference  between  it  and  the  simple  time  long.  This  seems 
to  be  the  case  with  Berger  and  Oattell,  who.  witli  a  simple  re- 
action time  of  147(7  and  150  >.  have  an  incomplete  re-action 
time  of  SOQ'J  and  277o~.  The  difference  between  the  incom- 
plete and  the  adaptive  re-action  seems  to  be  uniformly  small 
(many  of  the  differences  being  not  far  from  40(7),  though 
the  individual  variations  are  considerable.  It  is  likely  that 
the  effects  attributed  to  practice  and  fatigue  may  really  be 
due  to  a  change  from  the  sensory  to  the  motor  form  of  re- 
action. Thus  Kries  and  Auerbach  mention  that  their  incom- 
plete times  were  at  first  very  long,  but  that  they  became  very 
small,  the  reduction  continuing  long  after  the  effect  of  pi  tc- 
tice  upon  the  simple  re-action  had  ceased.  Again,  the  i'a<-t 


THE   TIME-RELATIONS   OP 


ft^ 

I 


^ 

^ 

-O 


-H 

4    S   1-  S3 


CO- 


I2 

CD-- 


1 

a 


33 

a 

•  ^     3  CD'  a        ^  •:  s  o 

"o 

CD 

bO 

c8 

Is- 

CD 

lafill 

2     &i>3  too-  -  --  -  -  - 

A 

S  §  1  §^  3 

a  -^  •  ho      o 
c  ^  ^r      aj 

2n  P3        0^        P^ 

CD       -    -                                          S" 
1/2 

>0                                                                 W 

©    3  '^*  '         m 

C5  Ed           ^^  ** 

CD     1 

CM  ^H  33     . 
O^2  CD   33 

o-i  i! 

i—  '  C-t  OJ  W  CJ  QI 

CD 
•«-> 

*fl 

a 

HH 

;;         - 

« 

•emu  UOUOB 

CO  ^O         O         £O  CO  'O  iO  »  "-O  £O 

§    S    P°gSgPgg    g 

OO             CM 

cc 

-9JJ  editing 

q:^    S    gS^SSi^s 

o      ^-       "^  *  —  r  —  ~'  ~^  —  o      co 

M         Tf         TT  •^•^t 

OOO             CO 
id  QO               OO 

.^ 

«CN      o<      rora  ..  cororoco 

c 

0 

CD 

IK 

s 

CD 

Ll 

CD 

4-1 
-     -                   CO 

O 

ta 

33 

0 

:3 

2    | 

i 

4> 

^e"0'^ 

» 

CD                                                             ' 

^5  ^^    -------- 

yj 

•* 

*    1 

T*                                                                        ^ 

l^J 

in 

l,| 

o  M  © 
o3  Cd  d 

2 
"a.      -      -  - 

CD                                                        3 

i;                               a  n 

^      -        --------'33 

1  -           1 

o     a 

g-            -            ------- 

o 

s 

I-H 

a      •          --------go 
(3                                                             0 
•O                                                            O 
•<                                                            ^^ 

*'                1 

|       ;  ;  i  ;  ;  ;  ; 

CO            

•     -o          

:  :     §  :'a 
•    •      £^    d 

1 

^  S      :    a  :  33  i  :  :  : 

S    |      :  ^2  :  :  :  :     a 

;    :      r  Os 

Nature  of  Distinc 
Between. 

"3                                K^            0     ^                          ; 

"i"^         O         O         <O  o  ~    **     U 

|a  1  E  iS"  gi 

fees       cS      i.      SB      2o 

_-        T3         *         "  ""         ^  " 

jjfl      a      CD      oa'cDm 

11  1 

M    «     £3^ad3:^l 

s  i  iigili  |  § 

D        U3        O  a^;^;  g'g     •             O 

>o     s-i    -  -  "2,-  g     o  5     M 
®    '3       '  cs"  5-  35    5 

PH       D^                  ^^       ^5       ^  £H       ^* 

o  Ca 

s-    1-1  r 

s 

a 
s 

d 
fc 

-a  5  a>?=5^ 

b^        ^~J  ij  i^  kj  f**-  J^  ij  p^        ^  j 

i  i  s 

MKNTAL   PHENOMENA. 


83!i  | 

1 

| 

M 

A 

M 

«M 

£ 

•?    .£3                ^               &( 

-~ 

•f. 

/.' 

cd 

£  ' 

Cl  -•    -    -• 

•;;•-  CD  •§      ~T 

; 

^ 

—  ' 

A 

CDHH 

i—  i 

-  i  8  _r    -5 

i 

-~ 

b 

IM 

^o 

13 

o!  *  - 

% 

n 

a  - 

u 

Cv 

a 

j., 

j-, 

a  a       °  §  fe 

S 

i 

0 

f, 

<*-< 

aa 

M       §|  * 

g-d 

5*  -  •£ 

®   1>    X     ^.    "•'"^ 

.  * 

:•: 

- 

o5    5 

5  ® 

fO              -t^     O     > 

•—i   nj 

«*H 
O           4J    3 

l^fllUiy 

i| 

-S 

g 

£ 
a 

=3    fe 

ti« 

O        -    -    - 

n  M  &JD  ^ 

o>                     „    9 

GO       - 

^ 

a 

0        ® 
tj)      -^ 

cs 

<d  a  a 

*.  H  «8 

o  a  a 
O 

6 
M 

"Ss  -                       ^co">' 
<D                          a 

-  -  -   <o 

-  -  -  -a 

H 

5 

<B 

3 

it         iO         ifl 

o 

s* 

o»  w 

« 

c?  w  ww  w 

?» 

TfClQ^         <?*         CC^O         Ol 

-reoooo 

\   \ 

a 

O5  Qj 

""-1 

03 

•d 

n 

a 

h-  ( 

CB 

eo        tra        5n 

| 

S 

ofc 

m 

COW  TPOOO 

•^  ^  co  »o  -^ 

JC 

»OCDCDO         ^         •^^'•^        GO 
t-X30CO         i~>        T—  IT—  tt—  i         GO 

'-f.  -J3330O 

OO  XJ3OCO 

\   \ 

ID 

tD         t-        CO 

gs 

— 
<— 

1 

ilo* 

T-« 

t-^aDC^Oi 
I-  £>  I-  O  «-H 

Tt   i-«*-l   WO? 

?> 

OXi-t~         CO         -fXTt*        (O 
OJ  TO  CO  CD         T-<          i—  i  r^T  rH         ol 

1-OS  ?!  X 

OS  00  CO  30 

c?  -r  in  in 

lO  t 

1O  C 

ft 
M 
CO 

ti 

a 

'   3 

«      2                        n 

_fl 

^rt 

S 

(H 

1        S           = 

a 

:= 

- 

I 

— 

1  Auerbai 
i< 

ii 

H 

:   "    ;    :    : 

<D  OQ 

•S-31 

"^ 

2                   S      g              2      | 

!"*si  rsi'i  I 

B    5         Is 

i  i  i  i 

"CB.J 

C3 

orrespouc 

S 

^^v 

_  Y—                                 v 

O 

u 

2 

S 

s 

•— 

&a 

a 

tn 

a 

s 

a 

•* 

to 

s  - 

Z 

-   -   *   ^   i 

•: 

iii-       ^       -ii       « 

-  i  I  3 

"g  J 

o 

a 

a 

0 

a 

lO 

ts. 

c1; 

ID 

si 

H 

<n 

4) 

ID 

> 

ID 

03 

4-j 

^ 

3, 

j) 

a 

** 

= 

•*  ~* 

" 

"   -   -   -   - 

o 

IV 

ft 

-  -  -  - 

5* 

-a 

•a 

§ 

K 

- 

09 

•d 

d 
o 

a 

o    •        •        • 

. 

1-1 

. 

r 

^ 

o 

2" 

0  :     S 

<n         •£ 

o-fi      S 

•I      :        ° 
a       .         CD     ^  ^  ^ 

a 

3 

p  a   £?  - 

St  3     te 

-     o 

*• 

03 

**       u  —  "* 

"5.      o-^o 

M  13                      " 

-i               a                        oo 

•a'S                             o 

a 

a! 

a 

aj      m 

a 

a 

^    in  ^ 

Ht! 

C5fi                   >H 

o 

oT.H       af 

~  %      •? 
•_  oj        y 

i? 
•  —  i 

o 

—  • 

£ 

3 

!jii®i 

SC  0  *  iS  - 

a 

2 

^  S-     -           i£         ---            03 
•  SB                                   03 

M    3                           ~™                                            O 

^  i  :  : 

? 

ga   o  - 
1$   * 

ID  -Z 
X  to     i        i 
O 

ft 
/. 

* 

Direction  o 

Colors  
Distance  of 

Localize  to 

2  •o'^Z  ° 

,_  d  tx  -p  t£ 
a  a  g  a  g 
H  E-I  -j?  f-  -Jo 

•a 
a 
d 
cc 

3 
^ 

^  CO   S  ®                                                ft 

3  S—  g     o                 g 
g  p.®  ft    -S     -  -  -      TS 

'    ®  fli        C3                             >•* 

^§5.5     g                  J3 

^  O  r~"  ^      V2                       1> 

i  i  "  ; 

88 

HH              t—  1        )—  1 
^               h-  (        1—  i 

M               >        > 

x      r;    x 

^      X 

X 

^ 

^ 

XXXIII 

IIIAXXX 

IIAXXX 
IAXXX 
AX  XX 
AIXXX 

XXXIX 

J  H-  —  .  i—  1            ^.            J^,._—            I_H 

M  y, 

P5 

34  THE  TIME-RELATIONS   OF 

that  simple  re-action  times  are  long  when  following  complex 
ones,  or  that  subjective  times  are  longer  when  following 
adaptive  re-actions,  seems  to  be  not  so  much  the  effect  of 
fatigue  as  of  a  continuance  of  a  sensory  mode  of  re-action. 
It  should  also  be  mentioned  that  Tigerstedt  ingeniously 
proposes  to  measure  the  distinction  time  by  taking  the  differ- 
ence between  two  incomplete  re-actions,  in  one  of  which  we 
re-act  to  a  definite  simple  impression,  and  in  the  other  to  the 
impression  requiring  distinction  (e.g.,  in  one  series  I  re-act 
to  white,  but  not  to  a  color;  in  the  other,  to  a  color,  but  not 
to  white);  and  the  difference  in  time  will  be  needed  for 
distinguishing  a  color  from  white.  The  general  fact  re 
mains,  then,  that  while  the  combined  distinction  and  choice 
times  exhibit  only  such  individual  and  other  variations  as 
seem  explicable  by  the  differences  in  the  conditions  of  ex- 
periment (the  adaptive  re  action  times  of  eight  of  Tischer's 
nine  subjects  fall  between  293  and  3205"),  the  estimates  that 
have  been  attempted  of  the  portions  of  the  time  due  to  dis- 
tinction and  to  choice  separately,  show  such  large  variations 
as  to  force  the  conviction  that  the  different  experimenters 
were  not  measuring  the  same  processes. 

Conditions  affecting  Distinction  and  Choice. 
Bearing  in  mind  that  we  are  dealing  with  comparative  re- 
sults only,  and  comparisons  restricted  mainly  to  the  results 
of  the  same  observer,  obtained  by  the  same  method,  we  pro- 
ceed to  investigate  the  conditions  by  which  these  processes 
involving  distinction  and  choice  are  affected.  It  will  be 
convenient  to  begin  with  the  effect  of  (1)  the  number  of  dis- 
tinctions and  of  choices.  The  effect  of  the  number  of  ob- 
jects among  which  distinction  is  to  take  place,  upon  the  time 
needed  to  make  the  distinction,  is  best  shown  in  the  "incom- 
plete" and  subjective  methods,  in  which  the  range  of  dis- 
tinction may  be  varied  without  affecting  that  of  choice. 
For  example:  Cattell  makes  an  incomplete  re-action  to  a 
certain  color  when  either  that  or  one  other  color  may  appear  in 
306o",  when  either  that  or  any  one  of  nine  other  colors  may 
appear  (IV  and  V.)  in  3130".  Friederich's  subjects  make  a 
subjective  distinction  bet  ween  two  colors  in  267<J,  between  four 


MENTAL    IMIKNOMENA. 


in  296<7  (XXXIX.  and  X  I,.).  Six  of  Tischer's  subjects  make 
a  subjective  distinction  between  two  sounds  of  different  inten- 
sity in  146(7  (simple  re-action,  114  (7)  ;  between  three  sounds, 
in  1640";  four  sounds,  in  178(7;  five  sounds,  in  194(7  (XLIV.- 
XLVII.).  Other  experiments  cited  in  the  table  (p.  32)  show 
the  same  slight  increase  of  distinction  time  with  the  increase  of 
the  range  of  impressions,  but  complicated  with  other  factors 
as  well.  With  regard  to  the  effect  upon  the  choice  time 
when  the  number  of  possible  choices  increases,  we  have  the 
results  of  Merkel,  who  found  for  the  simple  re-action  time  of 
ten  subjects  to  visual  impressions  1883";  for  an  adaptive  re- 
action between  two  impressions,  2760";  between  three,  3300"; 
between  four.  3940";  between  five,  4450";  between  six,  489(7; 
between  seven,  526(7;  between  eight,  562(7;  between  nine, 
581(7;  and  between  ten,  588(7  (partially  cited  in  XL  VIII.- 
L1I.).  The  impressions  were  the  numbers  1,  2,  3,  4,  5,  and  I., 
II.,  III.,  IV.,  and  V.  The  re-actions  to  movements  of  the  ten 
fingers  naturally  associated  with  these  impressions,  and  the 
naturalness  of  this  association  doubtlessly  contributes  to  the 
small  increase  in  time.  Miinsterberg  called  these  numbers 
and  re-acted  in  the  same  way,  finding  for  a  choice  between 
five  movements  383(7.  and  between  ten  478(7  (simple  re-action 
being  162(7).  Martins  finds  for  the  same  re-actions  474(7 
and  552(7  (average  of  three  observers).  It  being  established 
that  but  a  small  share  of  the  increase  is  due  to  the 
distinction  (Merkel  has  experimentally  shown  this  for 
his  subjects),  we  may  conclude,  that,  with  an  increase 
in  number,  the  difficulty  of  choice  increases  rr.ore 
rapidly  than  the  difficulty  of  distinction.  In  addition,  we 
have  reason  to  believe  that  the  increase  would  be  still  more 
marked  in  case  the  association  between  impression  and 
motion  is  artificial.  When  this  association  reaches  the 
maximum  of  natutalness,  in  naming  objects,  the  increase 
with  the  number  of  impressions  is  slight  Thus  it  may  !><• 
calculated  from  Cattell's  results  that  it  takes  him  but  a 


36  THE   TIME-RELATIONS  OF 

100"  longer  to  name  26  letters  or  short  words  than  to  name 
one  of  two,  but  QQff  longer  to  name  one  of  26  than  one  of 
two  pictures,  and  163(7  longer  to  name  one  of  ten  than  one 
of  two  colors;  the  action  of  naming  being  more  closely  re- 
lated to  letters  and  words  than  to  pictures  and  colors. 

It  is  the  ability  to  deal  promptly  and  correctly  with  a  large 
and  varying  number  of  impressions,  disposing  of  each  in  its 
appropriate  way,  that  we  recognize  as  evidence  of  mental 
power,  and  it  is  this  that  experiment  shows  to  be  a  factor 
of  great  influence  upon  the  time  of  an  adaptive  re-action. 
It  is  the  skill  in  disposing  of  so  large  a  number  of  adaptive 
re-actions  that  we  admire  in  the  post-office  clerk,  and  in  many 
other  exhibitions  of  manual  dexterity.  It  is  this  that  necessi- 
tates the  division  of  labor,  there  being  a  limit  to  the  number 
of  adaptive  re-actions  that  can  be  economically  controlled. 
Again:  the  fact  that  a  large  number  of  distinctions  does  not 
complicate  the  process  as  much  as  a  large  number  of  choices, 
finds  its  analogue  in  the  observation  that  our  power  of  re- 
production falls  below  our  powers  of  appreciation.  This 
plays  a  part  in  the  fact  that  we  learn  to  understand  a  lan- 
guage long  before  we  learn  to  speak  it,  and  in  many  similar 
processes.  The  development  of  mental  power  reveals  itself 
as  an  increasing  facility  in  performing  a  large  number  of 
complicated  adaptive  re-actions;  and  here,  too,  the  power 
of  appreciating  distinctions  develops  earlier  than  the  power  of 
choosing.  This  result  was  illustrated  experimentally  in  a 
brief  study  of  the  re-action  times  of  a  ten-year-old  child  as 
compared  with  those  of  an  adult.  While  the  pure  distinc- 
tion time  rose  from  58 ff  to  250(7  as  the  impressions  to  be 
distinguished  increased  from  two  to  five  'subjective  method, 
with  colors),  as  compared  with  44(f  and  78(7  for  adults,  for 
the  adaptive  re-action  for  two  impressions  the  time  was  120(7, 
for  five  impressions  603(7,  as  compared  with  79(7  and  210 ff 
for  adults. 

We  may  conveniently   introduce  the  general  topic  of  the 


MENTAL    PHENOMENA.  37 

effect  of  the  nature  of  the  distinction  and  the  choice  upon 
the  time  of  its  performance  with  ths  consideration  of  a  few 
points  affecting  th'e  distinction  alone.  (2)  The  similarity 
of  the  impressions.  The  endowment  of  the  various  sense- 
organs  varies  considerably  (e.g.,  the  sense  of  musical  pitch  is 
finer  than  that  of  sound  intensity) ;  but,  in  the  absence  of  a 
standard  of  comparison  of  sense-differences  in  disparate  types 
of  sensation,  we  can  only  illustrate  the  point  in  question  by 
varying  the  difficulty  of  distinction  within  the  same  sense. 
Thus  Kries  and  Auerbach  find  that  it  takes  much  longer  to 
tell  whether  a  sound  is  to  the  right  or  to  the  left,  according 
as  the  two  points  at  which  the  sound  is  produced  are  closer 
together.  When  they  form  an  angle  of  35°-120°  with  the 
centre  of  the  face,  the  additional  time  (by  the  incomplete 
method)  was  17 ff;  when  varied  between  35°  and  26°,  the 
time  was  78ff;  when  within  26°  and  11°,  it  was  137(7.  The 
ease  of  distinction  is  largely  a  function  of  practice.  We 
readily  seize  the  slight  optical  differences  furnished  by  the 
different  letters  of  a  known  language,  but  constantly  con- 
fuse much  greater  sense  differences  with  which  we  are  less 
familiar.  (3)  The  specific  nature  of  the  impression.  Very 
many  of  the  results  cited  in  the  table  may  be  said  to  illus- 
trate the  effect  of  a  change  in  the  nature  of  the  distinction ; 
but  it  is  difficult  to  show  this,  uncomplicated  with  other 
variations.  The  determinations  of  Kries  and  Auerbach 
(XXX.-XXXVIII.)  show  the  result  of  distinctions  of  vari- 
ous kinds,  though  an  analysis  of  the  causes  of  these  differ- 
ences is  hardly  practicable.  It  is  quite  clear  that  in  re-act- 
ing by  the  incomplete  method  the  re  action  is  shorter  when 
the  stimulus  is  the  stronger  of  t\vo  intensities  than  when  it 
is  the  weaker  of  the  two  (XXXVI  and  XXXVIII.).  Berger 
has  also  shown  that  the  intensity  of  the  stimulus  has  some 
influence  upon  the  distinction  time  beyond  what  would  be 
due  to  the  effect  upon  the  simple  re-action  time  therein  con- 
tained. The  difference  between  the  corresponding  simple 


;!S  THE   TIME-RELATIONS   OF 

and  the  incomplete  re-action  to  a  bright  light  is  85tf;  to  a 
medium  light,  119(J;  to  a  weak  light,  114(T;  while  similar 
differences  for  adaptive  re-actions  are  167<7,  179 ff,  1925";  the 
inference  being  that  the  intensity  of  the  stimulus  affects  the 
distinction  rather  than  the  choice.  Again  (in  the  series 
VI. -X.),  we  find  that  Cattell  recognized  most  quickly  that 
an  expected  one  of  26  pictures  was  present,  then  that  one  of 
26  letters,  next  one  of  28  short  English  words,  next  one  of 
26  short  German  words.  The  differences  between  the  time 
for  recognizing  letters  and  short  words  is  very  slight  com- 
pared to  the  increase  in  complexity  of  the  impression,  and 
thus  shows  the  effect  of  practice  in  recognizing  words  as  a 
whole.  Furthermore,  in  the  series  of  experiments  (partly 
cited  in  XLT.-XLIIF.)  in  which  one  to  six  place  numbers 
were  recognized,  while  there  is  a  concomitant  increase  in  the 
number  of  possible  impressions,  it  seems  fair  to  refer  the 
main  increase  in  time  to  the  increasing  complexity  of  the 
impression.  In  passing  from  the  recognition  of  one  to  two 
or  of  two  to  three  place  numbers,  the  increase  in  time  is 
slight;  but  from  there  on,  the  increase  itself  increases  with 
the  increase  of  the  number  of  numerals  (53 ff,  147 ff,  322<r),— 
a  fact  probably  related  to  the  practice  in  grasping  numbers 
in  groups  of  threes.  Another  series  (XXIV. -XXIX  )  may 
be  mentioned  here,  and  is  interesting  as  indicating  that  it  is 
more  difficult  to  tell  to  which  of  five  categories  (a  city,  a 
river,  etc.)  a  word  belongs  than  what  part  of  speech  it  is; 
and  this  is  in  turn  easier  than  to  tell  the  sphere  of  activity 
of  a  noted  man.  It  should  be  noted  that  the  choice,  the 
range  of  impressions,  the  connection  between  impression 
and  movement,  the  method  of  re-action,  are  equivalent  hi  all 
three  experiments;  so  that  the  difference  is  fairly  referable 
to  the  distinction  process  involved.  We  may  finally  notice 
as  here  pertinent  the  observations  of  Vintschgau  upon  the 
distinguishability  of  different  tastes.  He  found  that  by  the 
incomplete  method  it  took  longest  to  re-act  to  bitter  when 


MENTAL   PHENOMENA.  :>>!) 

the  alternative  \vas  distilled  water,  next  long  to  sweet,  next 
to  sour,  and  shortest  to  salt.  Similarly,  in  adaptive  re-ac- 
tions with  the  two  hands  to  all  possible  combinations  of  .two 
of  the  four  tastes,  salt  was  most  quickly  re-acted  to  (384(7), 
sour  next  (397(7),  sweet  next  (409(7),  and  bitter  last  (456(7). 

(4)  The  Foreknowledge  of  the  Subject.  Within  the  re- 
striction that  the  foreknowledge  of  the  subject  shall  be 
limited  to  the  knowledge  of  the  associative  boud  between 
stimulus  and  movement,  there  is  room  for  variation.  The 
simplest  case  would  present  but  one  stimulus  re-acted  to, 
and  but  one  not  re-acted  to,  or,  in  the  adaptive  re-action,  but 
one  stimulus  for  each  mode  of  re-action.  In  all  such  cases 
(I.,  II.,  IV.,  XI.,  XXXVIII.,  may  be  cited  as  instances) 
the  foreknowledge  of  the  subject  presents  the  maximum  of 
definiteness.  Any  departure  from  these  conditions  brings 
with  it  an  increase  in  the  time  of  re  action.  Cattell  finds 
but  a  very  slight  increase  (5-7(7)  in  the  incomplete  re-action 
when  the  stimulus  not  re-acted  fo,  instead  of  being  but  a 
single  one,  is  any  one  of  ten  colors,  but  finds  a  greater  in- 
crease (15(7,  difference  of  II.  and  III.)  when  the  stimulus 
re-acted  to,  instead  of  being  a  single  one,  is  one  of  ten 
colors,  though  the  particular  kind  of  color  need  not  be  recog- 
nized. Both  the  stimulus  re-acted  to  and  the  one  not  re- 
acted to  might  be  one  of  a  larger  or  smaller,  a  more  or  less 
homogeneous  group;  but  I  am  unable  to  find  a  rrcoid  of 
such  an  experiment.  The  somewhat  modified  form  of  ex- 
periment adopted  by  Tigerstedt  and  Bergqvist  shows  a  similar 
result.  They  re-acted  to  a  light,  when  either  the  light  or  a 
one  to  three  place  number  might  appear,  in  297<T,  and  to  the 
number  (including  its  recognition)  in  318(7.  If  the  number 
of  digits  of  the  numbers  that  may  appear  is  foreknown,  the 
time  is  considerably  reduced;  and  when  either  the  light  or  a 
foreknown  letter  might  appear,  the  time  for  recognizing  the 
light  was  still  further  shortened  (190(7).  The  same  scries  <>f 
variations  could  be  applied  to  adaptive  reactions  (i.e.,  one 


THE   TIME-RELATIONS   OF 


or  more,  or  all,  of  the  modes  of  re-action  might  be  associated 

with  any  member  of  a  variable  group  of  stimuli),  but  experi- 

ments designed  to  show  the  effect  of  such   variations  are 

lacking.      Mention  should  be  made,  however,  of  the  experi- 

ments of  Miinsterberg,  in  which  he  first  re-acts  witb  the  five 

fingers  to  five  categories,  each  limited  to  one  term  (XXIV. 

and  XXV.);  then  to  five  categories,  each  comprising  three 

terms    (XXVI.);    and    then    to  five  categories,    each   com- 

prising a  practically  indefinite  number   of  terms  (XXVII., 

XXVIII.,  XXIX.);  and  finds  an  increase  of  time  in  making 

these    steps    (Martius    has    repeated    experiments    XXV., 

XXVI.,  and  XXVII.,  finding  the  times  to  be  551ef,  644ff, 

and  804ff).      Although  other  factors  contribute   to   this  in- 

crease in  time,  part  of  it  may  be  referred  to  the  decreasing 

definiteness  of  the  foreknowledge  of  the  subject.     It  may  be 

added,  that  the   mechanism   by    which   an   increase  in   the 

number  of  possible  re-actions  increases  the  re-action   time 

is  allied  to  that  by  which  a  decrease  in  the  foreknowledge  of 

the  subject  does  so. 

The  effect  of  the  mode  of  re-action  upon  the  re-action 
time  is  the  same  here  as  in  the  simple  re-action.  Re-acting 
by  the  voice  in  the  incomplete  form  of  re-action  has  been 
found  to  be  longer  than  re-acting  by  the  finger;  and  when- 
ever the  re-action  takes  the  form  of  speaHng  or  naming,  it 
takes  some  time  to  place  the  organs  in  position  and  speak 
the  word.  But  a  very  special  and  important  effect  in  adap- 
tive re-actions  is  that  of  (5)  the  association  between  move- 
ment and  stimulus. 

As  the  effect  of  a  special  or  a  general  practice,  certain 
modes  of  re-acting  to  certain  types  of  stimuli  have  become 
natural,  easy,  and  familiar,  while  in  other  cases  (e.g.,  the 
re-acting  by  pressing  a  key,  —a  process  learned  only  for  the 
purposes  of  the  experiment)  the  association  is  extremely 
artificial  If  we  compare,  in  Miinsterberg's  series,  the  ex- 
periment in  which  the  five  fingers  re  act  to  the  numbers 


MENTAL  PHENOMENA.  41 

"one,"  "two,"  "three,'1  "four,"  "five"  (XXIV.),  with 
that  in  which  they  re-act  to  the  declensional  forms  of  a  Latin 
noun  (XXV.),  we  recognize  that  the  former  is  a  more  natu- 
ral association  than  the  latter,  and  seem  justified  in  at- 
tributing a  good  share  of  the  increase  in  time  to  this  differ- 
ence. Again:  to  re-act  by  naming  is  a  process  in  which  we 
have  had  considerable  training,  and  it  is  quite  evident  that 
the  time  needed  for  naming  one  of  26  different  impressions 
(XV.-XVJIL,  and  XX.)  is  much  shorter  than  would  be 
needed  for  re  acting  by  26  artificial  and  irregular  movements 
of  the  hand.  The  difficulty  in  learning  a  foreign  language, 
or  a  telegraphic  code,  or  a  shorthand  system  of  writing,  is 
largely  the  difficulty  of  forming  associations  between  com- 
plex stimuli  and  movements;  and  the  great  decrease  in  time 
that  is  brought  about  when  such  associations  have  been 
mastered  emphasizes  the  importance  of  the  factor  now  under 
discussion,  which,  in  turn,  may  be  regarded  as  an  expression 
of  the  effect  of  practice. 

We  may  push  the  analysis  a  step  farther.  The  process  of 
naming  is  much  more  closely  associated  with  a  word  or  a 
letter  than  with  a  picture  or  a  color;  for  the  former  are 
artificial  symbols,  becoming  significant  only  when  so 
interpreted,  while  the  latter  reveal  their  meaning  directly 
without  needing  to  be  named  or  read.  Accordingly,  we  find 
that  it  takes  longer  to  name  a  color  (6010")  or  a  picture 
(545(7)  than  to  name  a  letter  (4240")  or  a  word  (409(7),  though 
the  recognition  of  a  color  or  a  picture  is  a  quicker  process 
than  the  recognition  of  a  letter  or  a  word  (compare  XV., 
XVII..  XIX.,  XX.,  and  V.,  VI.,  VII.,  X  ).  Furthermore, 
if  the  time  of  naming  or  reading  is  thus  mainly  conditioned 
by  the  strength  of  association  involved,  we  may  in  turn 
utilize  this  process  as  an  index  of  familiarity  with  the  nam- 
ing or  reading,  or,  more  briefly,  with  the  language.  Thus 
Cattell,  an  American,  reads  English  words  more  quickly 
than  German  (XVII.  and  XVIIT.  t,  while  with  Berger,  a 


42  THE  TIME-RELATIONS  OF 

German,  this  relation  is  reversed.  To  name  a  picture  in 
German  occupies  Cattell  for  614(7;  in  English,  588 ff.  It 
occupies  Berger  in  German  for  501(7;  in  English.  580(7.  The 
inference  is  the  same  (though  the  absolute  time  is  much 
shorter)  if  we  read  words  in  construction  instead  of  isolated. 
By  this  method  Cattell  finds  that  he  can  read  an  English 
word  in  138(7,  a  French  in  167(7,  a  German  in  250(7,  an 
Italian  in  327(7,  a  Latin  in  434(7,  and  a  Greek  word  in  484(7, 
this  being  the  order  of  his  familiarity  with  these  languages. 
The  particular  nature  of  the  association  may  be  revealed  in 
the  study  of  these  time-relations.  Thus,  while  in  all  cases 
it  takes  longer  to  read  words  from  right  to  left  than  from 
left  to  right,  this  difference  is  relatively  least  in  the  least 
familiar  languages;  i.e.,  in  those  in  which  the  bond  of  asso- 
ciation between  the  words  is  least  significant.  For  a  like 
reason  letters  are  read  much  more  quickly  from  above  down- 
wards (102(7)  than  from  below  upwards  (264(7). 

(6)  The  Overlapping  of  Mental  Processes.  We  pass 
now  to  a  point  of  critical  importance  in  the  application  of 
results  gained  in  the  laboratory,  to  the  mental  operations  of 
daily  life.  While  in  the  former  case  we  are  performing  a 
set  task,  isolated  for  purposes  of  investigation,  in  the  lat- 
ter case  (i.e.,  in  such  operations  as  reading,  copying,  play- 
ing upon  instruments,  and  the  like)  we  are  performing  a 
continuous,  more  or  less  extended,  series  of  re-actions,  bound 
together  by  bonds  of  common  purpose  and  associations  of 
habit.  It  is  not  a  mere  aggregate,  but  an  organization  of 
mental  processes ;  and  this  makes  possible  the  performance 
of  the  several  factors  of  the  process  in  part  at  the  same  time. 
It  leads  to  an  "overlapping"  of  the  mental  elements.  It  is  a 
proficiency  in  thus  doing  several  things  at  once  that  consti- 
tutes much  of  the  difference  between  the  expert  and  the 
novice;  and  it  is  this  "telescoping"  process  that  seems  to  be 
the  method  by  which  complicated  operations  are  at  length 
performed  in  short  times.  It  is  for  this  reason  that  the  time 


MENTAL   PHENOMENA.  (:', 

per  word  of  reading  100  words  is  shorter  than  the  time  of 
reading  a  single  word.  Cattell  reads  a  short  word  in  4090", 
a  long  one  in  4510",  but  100  such  in  255(7  per  word,  and,  if 
the  words  are  in  construction,  in  125(T  per  word  ;  thus  indicat- 
ing how  much  of  the  difference  between  ordinary  reading,  and 
reading  single  words,  is  due  to  the  continuity  of  the  experi- 
ment, how  much  to  the  association  between  the  words.  So, 
also,  Cattell  reads  a  single  letter  in  424<7,  but  100  such  in 
224(7  (compare  XVII.,  XV.,  with  LIIL,  LIV.).  When  the 
series  is  too  long  continued,  fatigue  sets  in,  and  the  time  is 
again  longer;  it  is  longer  for  500  than  for  100  words  and 
letters;  and  for  colors  and  pictures  there  is  no  saving  in 
naming  100  above  naming  a  single  color  or  picture. 

A  special  study  of  this  power  of  grasping  several  things  at 
once  was  made  by  Cattell  by  having  letters  move  along  on 
the  surface  of  a  rotating  drum,  and  varying  the  width  of  a 
slit  in  a  screen  through  which  they  were  read.  When  the 
slit  just  allowed  one  letter  to  be  seen  at  a  time,  they  could 
be  read  at  the  rate  of  one  letter  in  228(7;  and  as  the  slit  was 
widened  to  admit  two,  three,  four,  five,  and  six  letters  at 
once,  the  rate  increased  to  one  letter  in  200(7,  178 (7,  166 ff, 
160(7,  and  160<7.  As  it  takes  424(7  to  name  a  letter  singly 
("XV.),  it  would  seem  that  the  whole  of  a  letter  need  not  be 
seen  at  once  to  be  recognized, — an  inference  corroborated  bv 
the  fact,  that,  when  the  slit  admils  only  one-tenth  of  a  let- 
ter at  a  time,  the  letters  can  be  read  at  4000"  per  letter.  The 
result  also  indicates  that  there  is  a  limit  to  the  power  in 
question.  M.  Paulhan  finds  similar  results  in  more  complex 
operations.  He  multiplies  numbers  and  recites  a  verse  or 
two  at  the  same  time ;  and  the  time  needed  for  this  is  shorter 
than  the  sum  of  the  times  required  to  do  each  separately. 
In  very  simple  cases  the  time  of  doing  both  together  is  not  longer 
than  the  time  for  doing  the  more  difficult  of  the  two  separately. 
The  mind  should  accordingly  not  be  likened  to  a  point  at 
which  but  a  single  object  can  impinge  at  our  time,  but 


44  THE  TIME-RELATIONS  OF 

rather  to  a  surface  of  variable  extension.  It  should  like- 
wise be  noted  that  the  performance  of  a  complex  and  ex- 
tended mental  task  is  not  the  same  thing  as  the  separate 
performance  of  the  several  elements  into  which  that  task 
may  be  analyzed. 

Mtinsterberg  has  applied  the  distinction  between  the 
sensory  and  motor  form  of  re-action  to  complicated  adaptive 
re-actions,  and  regards  it  as  there  involving  the  overlap- 
ping of  mental  processes.  The  times  cited  in  the  table 
(XXIV.-XXIX.)  in  Munsterberg's  experiments  refer  to 
sensory  re-actions.  In  these  the  attention  is  directed 
to  the  word  about  to  be  uttered.  It  is  recognized,  and  re- 
ferred to  its  group.  The  corresponding  movement  is  then 
aroused  and  performed,  the  several  processes  being  succes- 
sive in  time.  In  the  motor  form  the  word  is  thought  of  as  a 
"forefinger-moving"  word;  and  the  movement  upon  which 
the  attention  is  kept  fixed  is  expectantly  kept  ready  to  be 
set  off  at  the  slightest  notice.  The  several  processes  thus 
play  into  one  another,  some  perhaps  entirely  falling  away. 
Both  anticipatory  movements  and  errors  (moving  the  finger 
next  to  the  correct  one)  are  not  infrequent.  The  motor 
times  for  the  series  XX IV. -XXIX.  are  289,  355,  430,  432,  432, 
and  437 6;  the  differences  between  motor  and  sensory  times, 
94,  110,  258,  280,  461,  6850".  Until  these  very  important  and 
striking  results  are  better  understood,  it  would  be  unwise  to 
enter  into  a  discussion  of  them.  Martius  has  very  recently 
repeated  some  of  Munsterberg's  results  (XXLV.-XXVII.), 
and  not  only  fails  to  corroborate  them,  but  finds  that  the 
attention  to  the  movement  about  to  be  made  lengthens  the 
time.  He  holds  that  the  distinction  between  motor  and 
sensory  re-actions  is  applicable  only  to  simple  re-actions, 
and,  while  unable  to  explain  Munsterberg's  results,  does  not 
believe  the  difference  there  involved  to  be  the  same  as  that 
involved  in  simple  re-actions.  It  remains  for  future  research 
to  shed  light  upon  the  problem. 

(7)  Practice  and  Fatigue.  What  was  said  under  these 
headings  of  simple  re-actions  applies  with  equal  force  to 
complex  ones.  Various  experimenters  notice  the  decrease 
in  time  as  the  experiments  proceed.  They  note  that  this 


MENTAL   PHENOMENA.  C, 

decrease  is  relatively  greatest  at  first,  and  in  those  individu- 
als and  processes  whose  time  is  relatively  longest  at  the  out- 
set; also  that  it  soon  reaches  a  limit,  and,  when  once  thor- 
oughly acquired,  is  not  liable  to  be  lost  after  a  moderate 
degree  of  disuse;  and  that  it  at  times  seems  to  be  confused 
with  a  transition  from  a  sensory  to  a  motor  form  of  re-ac- 
tion. As  illustrative  of  one  or  other  of  these  points,  it  may 
be  mentioned  that  Tischer  finds  as  a  rather  typical  case  the 
decrease  of  a  distinction  time  from  1600"  in  the  first  set  to 
95(7  in  the  second,  and  860"  in  the  third,  all  reduction  ceas- 
ing on  the  average  after  5.5  sets;  that  Trautscholdt,  in  re- 
actions consisting  of  repeating  a  word,  finds  times  of  299, 
273,  and  258,  and  in  another  case  of  205,  176,  and  155 G,  in 
three  successive  periods  of  fourteen  days  each;  that  Ber- 
ger  and  Cattell,  beginning  with  some  practice  in  experi- 
ments of  this  kind,  find  the  time  for  incomplete  re-actions 
reduced  by  30  and  20(7  after  four  months'  experimentation; 
and,  finally,  that  the  great  decrease  in  the  incomplete  re- 
actions of  Kries  and  Auerbach  (from  64  and  117  to  21(7, 
from  153  and  109  to  36 ff,  from  104  and  97  to  49  and  54(7,  in 
various  experiments)  strongly  suggests  a  radical  change  in  the 
mode  of  re-action.  Another  aspect  of  the  effect  of  practice 
appears  in  a  study  by  Berger  of  the  times  required  by  the 
boys  of  the  nine  classes  of  a  German  Gymnasium,  and  of 
the  class  preparatory  to  the  Gymnasium,  to  read  100  and 
500  words  in  construction  in  German  and  in  Latin  at  a 
maximum  and  at  a  normal  rate.  There  is  a  constant  de- 
crease in  time  as  the  boys  advance  in  age.  In  Latin  the  several 
times  per  word  were  262,  135,  100,  84,  79,  57,  54,  49,  48,  43(7 ; 
in  German  72,  55,  43,  37,  39,  28,  27,  26,  25,  23(7;  the  great 
difference  between  the  first  two  times  in  Latin  being  due  to 
the  fact  that  the  boys  who  required  262(7  to  read  a  Latin 
word  had  never  learned  Latin  at  all.  That  these  differences 
are  to  be  referred  to  specific  practice  rather  than  to  general 
mental  maturity,  appears  from  a  comparison  of  the  above 


40  THE  TIME-RELATIONS  OF 

times  with  the  times  required  by  those  boys  to  name  colors; 
viz.,  135,  99,  119,  123,  100,  91,  112,  99,  86 ff. 

The  results  regarding  fatigue  are  not  equally  definite. 
Many  mention  the  general  fact  of  fatigue,  and  to  avoid  it 
perform  but  few  experiments  in  a  series.  We  have  already 
seen  that  it  takes  relatively  longer  to  read  500  letters,  words, 
colors,  pictures,  than  to  read  100.  On  the  other  hand,  Cat- 
tell,  after  a  very  long  series  of  re-actions,  found  no  serious 
or  constant  increase  in  the  time,  but  seemed  to  feel  the  ef- 
fects of  fatigue  on  the  following  day.  Both  practice  and 
fatigue  are  subject  to  large  individual  variations.  Oehrn 
has  studied  the  minor  variations  of  practice  and  fatigue  in  a 
session  of  two  hours1  work,  finding  first  a  stage  in  which 
practice  outweighs  fatigue,  and  then  a  stage  in  which  the 
reverse  is  true. 

(8)  Miscellaneous  and  Individual  Variations.  The 
complex  re-actions,  just  as  the  simple  ones  are  subject  to  the 
influences  of  distraction,  vary  under  the  action  of  drugs,  in 
morbid  conditions,  and  present  large  individual  variations. 
These  points,  though  frequently  noticed  incidentally,  have 
not  been  subjected  to  special  study,  so  that  briefly  citable 
and  conclusive  figures  are  lacking.  Regarding  the  action  of 
drugs,  Kraepelin  is  inclined  to  believe  that  the  distinction 
is,  under  their  influence,  almost  always  rendered  more  diffi- 
cult, being  only  slightly  subject  to  the  period  of  shortened 
times,  while  the  choice  factor  very  readily  becomes  shorter 
than  the  normal.  Marie  Walitzkaja  finds  that  the  complex 
re-action  times  in  the  insane  differ  more  from  the  normal 
than  do  their  simple  times.  __  An  adaptive  re-action  for  the 
two  hands  which  for  the  normal  required  351-406(7,  required 
707-9435"  in  cases  of  general  paralysis,  and  1085  ff  in  a  case 
of  mania.  These  should,  however,  be  regarded  as  individ- 
ual rather  than  general  results.  The  individual  variations 
may  be  regarded  as  increasing  with  the  complexity  of  the 
re-action.  Men  differ  more  from  one  another  in  the  time 


MENTAL   PHKNOMKXA.  47 

needed  for  doing  difficult  things  than  in  the  time  needed  for 
simple  things.  Systematic  experimentation  upon  this  point 
is  lacking:  but  a  suggestion  of  the  truth  may  be  obtained  by 
calculating  the  average  deviation  from  their  mean,  of  Mer- 
kel's  ten  subjects  in  their  simple  re-action  times,  their  sub- 
jective distinction  times,  and  their  adaptive  re-action  times; 
the  result  being  2.23  per  cent,  3.35  per  cent,  and  6.79  per 

cent. 

Association  Times. 

While  the  effect  of  the  association  between  stimulus  and 
movement  upon  the  time  of  the  re-action  has  been  already 
discussed,  the  process  of  association  forms  so  important  a. 
factor  in  our  mental  life,  that  it  requires  a  more  specialized 
and  independent  investigation. 

(1)  Questions  ivith  but  a  Single  Ansiver.  We  may 
view  an  adaptive  re-action  under  the  aspect  of  a  "question 
and  answer;"  the  stimulus  being  equivalent  to  the  question, 
'What,  with  regard  to  certain  points,  is  this  impression?" 
and  the  answer,  whether  indicated  by  a  name,  or  word,  or 
movement,  is  given  in  the  re-action.  Our  problem  is  to  in- 
vestigate the  time-relations  of  these  questions  and  answers, 
as  an  index  of  the  readiness  of  the  association  between  the 
two.  The  processes  intervening  between  the  appreciation  of 
the  question  and  the  formulation  of  the  answer  may  vary 
greatly  in  complexity  and  character.  A  common  character- 
istic of  the  re-actions  hitherto  regarded  consisted  in  the  fact 
that  the  material  for  forming  the  answer  is  simply  and  di- 
rectly supplied  by  the  stimulus  itself:  it  is  in  the  main  a 
verdict  regarding  the  particular  nature  of  sensation  then 
present.  The  re-actions  to  which  we  now  pass  all  include 
something  more  than  this;  and  the  formulation  of  the  an- 
swer involves  to  a  greater  or  less  extent  more  complicat>-:l 
forms  of  mental  activity,  and  depends  more  or  less  upon  the 
past  experiences,  the  special  habits  and  tendencies  of  mind, 
of  the  individual. 


48  THE  TIME -RELATIONS  OF 

While  the  line  of  division  between  the  direct  appreciation 
and  the  indirect  interpretation  of  a  sense-impression  cannot 
be  rigidly  drawn,  and  while  it  is  no  less  difficult  to  decide 
what  processes  are  involved  in  this  interpretation  and  elabo- 
ration of  the  sense-impression,  yet  we  may  with  sufficient 
precision  mark  out  as  the  first  class  of  associations  (a)  those 
in  which  a  simple  act  of  memory  plays  the  chief  role. 
Thus,  when  Cattell,  instead  of  naming  a  picture  in  his  own 
language  (which  he  does  in  545(7),  names  it  in  German  (in 
6940"),  the  difference  in  time  is  needed  for  calling  to  mind 
the  German  name,  and  measures  the  strength  of  this  asso- 
ciation. Berger's  acquaintance  with  English  is  less  than 
CattelTs  with  German,  and  accordingly  with  him  the  differ- 
ence between  naming  a  picture  in  the  vernacular  and  in  a 
foreign  language  is  greater  (477 ff  and  649(7).  The  transla- 
tion of  a  short  familiar  word  from  English  to  German  occu- 
pies Cattell  686(7;  from  German  to  English,  but  5SO<J;  the 
time  for  long  and  less  familiar  words  being  much  longer. 
(We  may  obtain  the  portion  of  the  time  required  for  the  act 
of  translation  alone  by  subtracting  from  this  the  time  to  see 
and  name  a  word,  428(7.)  Such  operations  as  addition  and 
multiplication,  when  confined  to  numbers  of  one  place,  can 
hardly  be  more  than  acts  of  memory.  Cattell  adds  such 
numbers  in  336(7;  Berger,  who  is  a  mathematician,  in  221  ff. 
The  former  multiplies  them  in  544^7;  the  latter,  in  389(7. 
Vmtschgau's  three  subjects  multiply  such  numbers  (though 
under  different  conditions)  in  233(7.  More  complicated  types 
of  "memory  re-actions"  have  been  performed  by  Cattell  and 
by  Miinsterberg.  The  former  determined  in  separate  series 
the  time  necessary,  when  given  a  city,  to  name  the  country 
in  which  it  was  situated  (462(7);  when  given  a  month,  to 
name  the  season  to  which  it  belongs  (310(7),  to  name  the  fol- 
lowing month  (389(7),  to  name  the  preceding  month  (832(7); 
given  an  author,  to  name  the  language  in  which  he  wrote 
(350(7) ;  given  an  eminent  man,  to  name  his  sphere  of  activity 


MENTAL  PHENOMENA.  49 

(368(7).  Miinsterberg  constantly  varied  the  type  of  question 
including  suck  as  the  above,  the  position  of  cities,  the  quali- 
ties of  objects,  the  relations  of  men,  and  many  others,  find- 
ing an  average  time  of  848(7  (average  of  two  subjects). 
While  many  of  these  determinations  are  doubtless  of  more 
individual  than  general  value,  we  may  stop  to  note  a  few 
points  that  are  presumably  typical.  The  re-actions  here 
grouped  under  one  class  vary  considerably  in  difficulty,  and 
a  few  instances  may  be  cited  to  indicate  the  range  of  this 
variation.  In  giving  a  country  in  which  a  given  city  is 
situated,  the  shortest  time  is  for  Paris  (278ff) ;  the  longest, 
for  Geneva  (4850").  In  giving  the  language  in  which  an  au- 
thor wrote,  Berger  requires  least  time  for  Luther  (227(7)  and 
Goethe  (265(7),  most  for  Bacon  (565(7);  Cattell,  least  for 
Plato  (224(7)  and  Shakspeare  (258(7),  most  for  Plautus 
(478(7).  In  giving  the  calling  of  an  eminent  man,  the  least 
time  is  required  for  poets  (291(7),  the  longest  for  men  of 
science  (421(7).  Miinsterberg  mentions  as  quickly  answered 
questions  (400(7  to  600(7),  "On  what  river  is  Cologne?"  "  In 
what  season  is  June?"  "In  what  continent  is  India?"  as 
questions  requiring  a  long  time  (1100(7  -1300(7),  "Who  is 
the  author  of  Hamlet?"  "What  is  the  color  of  ice?"  "Who 
was  the  teacher  of  Plato?"  An  influence  which  we  have 
found  of  great  significance  hitherto  is  equally  important 
here;  viz.,  the  foreknowledge  of  the  subject  of  what  is  to 
occur.  In  Cattell's  experiments  the  general  question  is  vir- 
tually asked  once  for  the  entire  series,  the  special  terms  be- 
ing given  in  each  experiment,  while  in  Miinsterberg's  results 
the  entire  question  changes  with  each  observation;  and  this 
difference  in  the  expectancy  of  the  subject  cannot  but  be  an 
important  factor  in  the  longer  times  found  by  the  latter.  A 
somewhat  different  phase  of  this  influence  appears  in  the 
results  of  Vintschgau.  In  multiplying  the  numbers  from 
1X1  to  9X9,  the  smaller  number  was  always  announced 
first.  Accordingly,  when  the  first  nine  was  announced,  the 


50  THE  TIME-RELATIONS   OF 

subject  practically  anticipated  the  result,  and  had  the  prod- 
uct ready;  when  eight  was  announced,  he  knew  that  it  was 
one  of  two  results;  when  seven,  one  of  three;  and  so  on. 
Accordingly  we  find  these  to  be  the  shortest  processes  (9x9, 
only  160(5")  but  there  is  another  factor  at  work  counteract- 
ing this  effect,  viz.,  the  familiarity  of  certain  multiplications, 
making  the  products  by  one  short,  and  those  by  four  and 
five  long. 

(b)  The  next  type  of  "question  and  answer11  will  be  one 
in  which,  in  addition  to  the  act  of  memory,  a  comparison, 
or  a  judgment,  is  involved.  The  result  of  the  comparison, 
thougb  not  always  the  same  for  all  individuals  (and  in  this 
sense  the  question  is  not  limited  to  a  single  answer),  will 
probably  always  be  the  same  in  the  same  individual.  The  only 
experiment  of  Cattail's  that  seems  properly  to  belong  here  is 
that  in  which  the  subject  decided  which  was  the  greater  of 
two  eminent  men  (558(7).  Munsterberg  finds  the  average 
time  for  answering  a  miscellaneous  group  of  such  compari- 
sons 947(7,  or  99(7  longer  than  the  process  without  compari- 
son: comparisons  rapidly  made  (600(7-800(7)  being,  "Which 
has  the  more  agreeable  odor, — cloves  or  violets?''  "Who  is 
greater, — Virgil  or  Ovid?"  "What  is  more  beautiful, — woods 
or  mountain?"  and  difficult  questions  (1200(7-1500(7)  being, 
"Which  is  healthier, — swimming  or  dancing?"  "Which  do 
you  like  better, — Goethe's  drama  or  his  lyric?"  "Which  is 
more  difficult, — physics  or  chemistry?"  The  comparison 
may  be  among  more  than  two  objects.  Thus,  in  asking  which 
is  the  finest  of  Goethe's  dramas,  the  process  of  formulating 
the  reply  may  include  the  calling  to  mind  what  the  various 
dramas  are,  and  a  choice  among  them;  not,  of  course,  a  con- 
siderate judgment,  but  the  selection,  under  the  necessity  of 
an  immediate  answer,  of  one  deciding  motive.  On  the  other 
hand,  among  the  several  possibilities,  a  certain  one  may,  by 
habitual  association  or  for  other  reasons,  have  become  &o 
prominent  that  virtually  no  comparison  ensues;  and  the 


MENTAL   PHENOMENA.  51 

relatively  slight  excess  in  time  of  this  type  of  association 
above  the  former  ones  (1049<7)  suggests  that  this  was  often 
the  case.  To  decide  which  is  the  pleasantest  odor  (rose),  or 
which  the  most  important  German  river  (Rhine),  required 
only  between  6000"  and  700(7;  to  decide  which  was  the  most 
difficult  Greek  author  (Pindar),  or  your  favorite  French 
writer  (Corneille),  from  14000-  to  1600(7. 

Miinsterberg  has  ingeniously  modified  this  form  of  experi- 
ment to  show  the  influence  of  the  foreknowledge  or  pre- 
paredness of  the  subject.  He  precedes  the  asking  of  the 
question  by  a  dozen  or  so  words  of  the  category  within 
which  the  comparison  is  to  be  made.  Thus,  ''Apples,  pears, 
cherries,  peaches,  plums,  grapes,  strawberries,  dates,  figs, 
raisins:  which  do  you  like  better, — grapes  or  cherries?" 
Although  the  comparison  cannot  be  begun  until  the  last 
word  is  heard,  still  the  subject  has  in  a  way  anticipated  the 
general  nature  of  the  question,  as  well  as  the  scope  of  the 
comparison,  and  has  reduced  the  time  considerably  (676(7,  as 
compared  with  947(7), — certainly  a  striking  result. 

(2)  Questions  with  More  than  a  Single  Answer.  In  the 
class  of  re-actions  to  which  we  now  pass,  the  question  ad- 
mits of  several  answers.  The  answer  at  one  time  may 
not  be  the  same  as  at  another  time;  and  the  determining 
factors  in  the  particular  character  of  the  answer  are  the 
peculiar  mental  habits  and  tendencies  of  the  individual. 
The  question  thus  changes  from  a  specific  to  a  general  one, 
the  answer  being  any  member  of  a  more  or  less  extended 
class  answering  to  such  and  such  a  description.  In  some 
the  choice  may  be  somewhat  limited.  This  is  true  of  Cat- 
telFs  experiments  in  which,  given  a  country,  we  are  to  name 
a  city  in  it  (346(7);  given  a  season,  to  name  a  month  in  it 
(435ff);  given  a  language,  to  name  an  author  writing  in 
that  language  (519(?) ;  or,  given  an  author,  to  name  any  work 
of  his  (763(7).  In  all  these  cases  we  are  apt  to  have  in  mind 
only  a  very  few  prominent  instances  under  each  head  among 


52  THE   TIME-RELATIONS   OF 

which  individual  preference  is  exercised.  In  the  following 
series  the  classes  are  more  general,  and  accordingly  the 
scope  for  individual  preference  much  larger:  given  a  general 
term  to  name  a  particular  instance  under  that  term  (537 ff) ; 
given  a  picture  to  name  some  detail  of  it  (447 ff) ;  given  the 
word  instead  of  the  picture,  to  make  a  similar  association 
(439(7)  ;  given  the  picture  or  the  name  to  mention  some  prop- 
erty of  it  (372(7  and  337(7) ;  given  a  quality  to  name  an  ob- 
ject to  which  it  can  be  applied  (351(7);  given  an  intransitive 
verb  to  find  an  appropriate  subject  (527(7),  or  a  transitive 
verb  to  find  an  appropriate  object  (379<J).  Miinsterberg  has 
a  series  including  a  miscellaneous  collection  of  such  re-ac- 
tions, and  finds  a  time  of  1036(7.  Trautscholdt  has  investi- 
gated a  similar  series  in  which  a  specific  instance  of  a  gen- 
eral term  had  to  be  given,  and  finds  a  time  of  1020(7  (average 
of  three  subjects),  155(7  of  which  must  be  deducted  to  get  the 
pure  association  time. 

Here,  again,  we  may  stop  to  consider  a  few  generalizations 
which  these  results  seem  to  sustain.  The  processes  involved 
vary  very  considerably  in  the  different  experiments.  Miin- 
sterberg cites  as  quick  responses  (450(7-600(7)  the  instancing 
of  '  a  German  wine  (Rudesheimer),"  "of  a  number  between 
ten  and  four  (six),'  "of  a  Greek  poet  (Homer);"  as  slow 
ones  (1200(7-1500(7),  "a  beast  of  the  desert  (lion),1'  "  a  French 
author  (Voltaire)."  Trautscholdt  names  "  mast  "as  "a  part 
of  a  ship"  in  391(7,  but  requires  1899(7  to  name  "art"  as 
'  an  aesthetic  activity  of  man."  These  differences  should  ap- 
pear in  the  average  variations:  that  is,  the  average  divergence 
of  the  re-action  times  from  their  mean.  When  the  process 
is  simple  and  constant,  the  average  variation  is  small ;  when 
the  processes  are  complicated  and  variable,  the  average 
variation  is  large.  While  in  simple  re-actions  it  is  often  less 
than  10  per  cent  of  the  re-action  time,  it  is  not  infrequently  as 
high  as  30  per  cent  in  the  re-actions  just  considered.  It  may 
have  been  noticed  that  in  certain  cases  the  process  in  (2)  was 


MENTAL   PHENOMENA.  53 

the  reverse  of  that  in  (1).  The  one  was  a  step  from  the 
whole  to  the  part,  the  general  to  the  special;  while  the  other 
was  from  the  part  to  the  whole,  the  special  to  the  general. 
In  CattelTs  case 'the  former  is  the  longer  (433(7  and  374(7). 
In  Trautscholdt's  results  the  conclusion  comes  out  more 
clearly,  the  pure  association  time  of  an  association  of  part  to 
whole  is  608(7;  of  whole  to  part,  901<7;  of  special  to  general, 
754(7;  of  general  to  special,  947(7.  It  is  thus  easier  to  refer 
an  individual  ohject  or  quality  to  its  class  than  to  give  an 
instance  of  a  general  concept.  A  similar  result  (namely, 
that  the  bond  of  association  between  two  concepts  is  not 
equally  strong  in  both  directions)  is  derived  from  observing 
that  it  takes  longer  to  recall  that  May  precedes  June  than 
that  June  follows  May,  longer  to  go  back  and  find  a  subject 
for  a  verb  than  to  go  forward  and  find  an  object  for  it,  longer 
when  given  a  quality  to  find  an  object  possessing  that  quality 
than  to  recall  a  quality  for  an  object,  and  so  on. 

We  may  here  also  conveniently  consider  the  overlapping 
of  mental  processes,  which  we  have  found  takes  place  when- 
ever a  series  of  simple  processes,  or  a  complex  process  in- 
volving many  simple  ones,  is  performed.  The  general  truth 
that  the  time  of  a  complex  mental  operation  is  less  than  the 
sum  of  the  times  needed  for  the  performance  of  the  separate 
factors  into  which  the  former  may  be  resolved,  will  be  again 
illustrated.  Thus  Mtinsterberg  finds  that  it  takes  103(7  to 
name  a  specific  instance  of  a  class  (e.g.,  to  name  a  German 
river),  992(7  to  make  a  comparison,  (e.g..  Which  is  more  im- 
portant,—  this  river  or  that?)  but  only  1049(7  to  decide 
both  questions  together  (e.g..  Which  is  the  most  important 
German  river  ?)  In  this  case  we  clearly  recognize  that  the 
last  pi'ocesses  are  not  the  sum  of  the  preceding  two,  but  that 
the  category  "most  important  German  river"  is  already 
formed  in  the  mind.  The  following  comparisons  are  more 
illustrative.  Instead  of  asking  first,  "Which  is  the  most 
important  German  river  ?"  (1049(7,)  and  then,  "  Which  lies 


54  THE  TIME-RELATIONS  OF 

more  westerly, -- Berlin,  or  the  Rhine?1'  (992(7,)  we  ask 
at  once,  "Which  lies  more  westerly,  -- Berlin,  or 
the  most  important  German  river  ? "  and  find  the  time 
18550",  or  176  ff  less  than  the  sum  of  the  two  foregoing 
processes.  Similarly,  if  instead  of  asking  first,  "On  what 
river  is  Cologne  situated?"  (848 ff,)  and  then,  "Which  is 
more  westerly,  —  the  Rhine  or  Berlin  ?"  (992(7,)  we  ask  at 
once,  "Which  is  more  westerly, — Berlin,  or  the  river  on 
which  Cologne  is  situated  ? "  we  find  a  more  remarkable 
saving  of  time  (1314(7,  or  526(7  less  than  the  sum  of  the  two 
questions).  This  time  was  still  further  reduced  to  1149<T 
when  the  question  was  preceded  by  a  list  of  a  dozen  cities. 

(3)  Unlimited  Associations.  When  we  pass  to  the  re- 
action of  naming  as  rapidly  as  possible  any  word  whatever, 
that  is  suggested  by  a  given  word,  we  are  drawing  entirely 
upon  the  natural  associative  habits  of  the  individual,  and 
accordingly  this  method  has  been  most  useful  in  studying 
psychological  habits  and  tendencies.  Our  present  purpose, 
however,  is  only  with  the  time-relations  of  this  unrestricted 
association.  This  has  been  the  type  of  association  first  and 
most  frequently  investigated,  and  it  is  customary  to  speak  of 
the  "  pure  association  time  "  as  the  total  time  minus  the  time 
needed  to  repeat  a  word.  Thus  Miinsterberg  repeats  a  word 
in  3820",  and  calls  out  a  word  in  association  with  the  given 
word  in  896 ff.  Trautscholdt,  however,  who  experimented 
upon  Wundt,  Stanley  Hall,  and  two  other  subjects,  finds  an 
average  time  of  1024(7,  727(7  of  which  is  regarded  as  the 
pure  association  time.  Galton  and  others  have  made  esti- 
mates, by  rougher  methods,  of  the  rapidity  with  which 
trains  of  ideas  pass  through  the  mind,  and  the  result  is  a 
rate  not  differing  much  in  either  direction  from  one  associa- 
tion per  second.  It  will  be  recognized  at  once  that  this  pro- 
cess will  be  very  different  in  different  individuals  and  with 
different  words.  Miinsterberg's  shortest  association  was 
"gold-silver"  (390(7);  the  longest,  "sing-dance,"  "moun- 


MENTAL    PHENOMENA.  -Vi 

tain-level''  (1100(7-14000-).  Trautscholdt  also  found  "  gold- 
silver  "  a  very  quick  re-action  (4020-),  "  storm-wind  "  (368(7), 
"  duty-right"  (415(7).  Long  re-actions  were  "  God-fearing  " 
(1132(7),  "throne-king"  (1437(7),  "  Karl- August"  (1662(/). 
Some  interesting  inferences  result  from  the  consideration  of 
the  times  of  different  types  of  these  unrestricted  associations 
Trautscholdt  divides  these  into  "  word  associations,"  or  those 
suggested  hy  the  word  rather  than  by  the  thing;  "outer 
associations,"  or  those  relating  to  the  sense-qualities  of  the 
object;  and  "inner"  or  logical  associations.  The  results 
were  1033;7,  1028(7,  989(7,  though  this  order  may  be  liable  to 
individual  differences.  Cattell  and  Berger  have  also  com- 
pared the  re-action  times  to  concrete  nouns  (374(7,  pure  asso- 
ciation time),  to  less  concrete  nouns  (462(7),  to  abstract  nouns 
(570(7),  and  to  verbs  (501(7),  clearly  showing  that  concrete 
terms  are  more  readily  suggestive  than  abstractions,  and 
concrete  objects  more  so  than  actions.  Trautscholdt  finds 
for  associations  to  concrete  nouns,  710(7;  to  actions,  837(7;  to 
abstractions,  871(7. 

Many  of  the  influences  to  which  we  found  simpler  forms 
of  re-action  times  open,  are  doubtless  true  of  association 
times,  but  the  great  variability  of  the  latter  makes  these  diffi- 
cult to  establish.  The  effect  of  practice  is  noticed  by  Traut- 
scholdt; and  Cattell  has  shown  that  in  students  from  thirteen 
to  eighteen  years  of  age  a  distinct  shortening  of  the  associa- 
tion time  accompanies  growth  and  education,  while  the 
students  ranking  higher  in  class  have  a  somewhat  shorter 
time  than  those  standing  low  in  class.  Fatigue  very  readily 
enters,  the  accessible  associations  are  easily  exhausted,  ;m<l 
the  mind  repeats  itself  very  markedly.  Changes  under  the 
action  of  drugs  and  in  morbid  mental  states  have  been  inci- 
dentally noticed,  but  still  await  systematic  investigation. 

The  various  processes,  the  times  of  which  we  have  been 
studying,  by  no  means  exhaust  the  possibilities  in  this  field. 
As  our  knowledge  of  mental  operations  becomes  more  per- 


56  THE   TIME-RELATIONS  OF   MENTAL   PHENOMENA. 

feet  and  more  capable  of  experimental  study,  and  as  our 
power  of  analysis  makes  similar  progress,  the  study  of  the 
time-relations  of  mental  phenomena,  already  fertile  in  sug- 
gestions and  results,  will  increase  in  interest  and  impor- 
tance. 


BIBLIOGRAPHY. 

(This  bibliography,  though  intended  to  be  fairly  complete,  is  by  no 
means  exhaustive.  It  is  furthermore  Limited  to  the  points  discussed  in  the 
text,  and  the  classification  corresponds  in  the  main  with  the  order  of  topics 
in  the  text.  Each  article  is  cited  in  full  under  the  topic  to  which  it  is  more 
especially  devoted,  and  referred  to  by  number  under  other  topics  to  which  it 
contributes  to  a  greater  or  less  extent.  While  the  general  articles  are  fre- 
quently cited  under  the  special  topics,  it  will  be  well  to  consult  them  even 
when  not  so  cited.  Specially  important  articles  have  their  numbers  printed 
in  bold  type.) 

A.  GENERAL. 

I.  Expository. 

I.  WUNDT.    Physiologische  Psychologic,  3d  ed.,  1887,  vol.  ii.  pp.  261-364. 
2.  SERGI.    La  Psychologic  Physiologique.  1888,  pp.  272-296. 

3.  BUCCOLA.    La  legge  del  tempo  nei  fenomeni  del  pensiero,  1883. 
4.  LADD.    Elements  of  Physiological  Psychology,  1887,  pp.  468-497. 

5.  KRAEPELIN.    BiolofjischesCentralblatt.  vol.  i.  (several  articles). 

6.  KRAEPELIX.    Die  Neueste  Literatur  auf  dem  Gebiete  der  psychischen 

Zeitmessung,  Idem,  Hi.  53-63. 

7.  FRICKE.    Ueber  psychische  Zeitmessung,  Idem,  viii.  673-690;  ix.  234-256, 

437-448,  467-469. 
8.  BIBOT.    German  Psychology  of  To-day  (translation),  1886,  pp.  250-287. 

9.  EXNER.    Herrmann's  Handbuch  der  Physiologic,  ii.  2,  part  ii.  chap.  iv. 

pp.  252-283. 

ii.  Experimental. 

10.  DONDERS.  Die  Schnellegkeit  Psychischer  Processe(DuBois-Reymond^ 

Archiv,  1868,  pp.  657-681). 

II.  DE  JAAGER.    De  physiologische  Tigo  bij  psychische  Processen,  Thesis, 

1865. 

12.  KRIES  and  AUERBACH.     Die  Zeitdauer  einfachster  psychische   Vor- 

gange  (Du  Bois-Reymond's  Archiv,  1877,  pp.  297-378). 

13.  CATTELL.     Psychometrische  Untersuchungen  (Wundfs  Studien,  iii. 

pp.  2,  3;  also  as  thesis,  1886). 

14.  JASTROW.    An  Easy  Method  of  Measuring  the  Time  of  Mental  Processes 
(Science,  Sept.  10,  1886). 

B.  SIMPLE  RE-ACTIONS. 

i.  General. 

15.  EXNER.    Experimentelle  Untersuchungen  der  einfachsten  psychischen 

Processe  (Pfliif/ci-'x  Anitir,  vii.  601-060,  viii.  520-537,  xi.  in:!   I  :.' 

57 


58  THE   TIME-RELATIONS  OF 

ii.  Inertia  of  Sense-Organs. 

16.  CATTELL.    The  Inertia  of  the  Eye  and  Brain  (Brain,  1885). 

17.  PEEYER.    Grenzen  des  Empfindungsvermogens,  etc.,  1868. 

18.  SANFORD.    The  Relative  Legibility  of  the  Small  Letters  (American 

Journal  of  Psychology,  i.  pp.  402-436). 

19    EUNKEL.    Ueber  die  Abhangigkeit  der  Farbenempflndung  von  der  Zeit 
(Pfluger"s  Archiv,  ix.  pp.  197-220). 

20.  V.  WITTICH.    Bemerkungen  zu  Preyer's  Abhandlung  (Idem,  ii.  pp.  329- 

350). 

21.  BAXT.    Ueber  die  Zeit,  welche  nothig  ist  damit  ein  Gesichtseindruck 

zum  Bewusstsein  kommt,  etc.  (Idem,  iv.  pp  325-336). 

22.  EXNER.    Ueber  die  zu  einer  Gesichtswahrnehmung  nothige  Zeit  (Wie- 

ner Sitzungsberichte,  58,  pp.  601-631). 
47. 

in.  Be  actions  to  Impressions  of  Sight. 
10,  11,  13,  15,  29,  31,  48. 

23.  VINTSCHGAU    and   LUSTIG.     Zeitmessende    Beobachtungen    iiber    die 

Wahrnehinung  des  sich  entwickelnden  positiven  Nachbildes  eines 
electrischen  Funkens  (Pfliiger's  Archiv,  xxxiii.  pp.  494-512). 

IV.  Re-actions  to  Impressions  of  Sound. 
10,  13,  15,  33,  45,  49. 

v.  Re-actions  to  Impressions  of  Touch. 

24.  VINTSCHGAU.    Die  physiologische  Reactionszeit  und  der  Ortsinn  der 

Haut  (Pfluger's  Archiv,  xxii.  pp.  87-110). 
10,  11,  12,  15,  26,  27,  31,  32,  37. 

v.  (a)  Temperature. 

25.  GOLDSCHEIDER.    Du  Bois-Reymond's  Archiv,  1887.  p.  469. 

26.  VINTSCHGAU  and  STEINACH.    Zeitmessende  Versuche  iiber  den  Tem- 

peratur-  und  Drucksinn  (Pfliiger^s  Archiv,  xliii.  pp.  152-195). 

vi.  Re-actions  to  Taste. 

27.  VINTSCHGAU  and  HONIGSCHMIED.     Versuche    iiber  die  Reactionszeit 

einer  Geschmacksempfindung  (Pfl tiger's  Archiv,  x.  pp.  1^18,  xiv.  pp. 
529-592). 

vn.  3e  actions  to  Smell. 

28.  MOLDENHAUER.    Ueber  die  einfache  Reactionszeit  einer  Geruchsem- 

pflndung  (Wundfs  Studien,  i.  pp.  606-614). 

vni.  Influence  of  Intensity  of  Stimulus. 

29.  BERGER.    Ueber  den  Einfluss  der  Reizstiirke  auf  die  Dauer  einfacher 
psychischer  Vorgange,  etc.  (WundVs  Studien,  iii.  pp.  38-93). 

30.  CATTELL.    Influence  of  the  Intensity  of  the  Stimulus  on  the  Length  of 

the  Re-action  Time  (Brain,  1886,  pp.  512-516). 
12,  15,  32,  49. 

ix.  Variation  of  Stimulus  within  the  Same  Sense. 

31.  HALL  and  KRIES.    Ueber  die  Abhangigkeit  der  Reactionszeit,  etc.  (Du 

Bois-Reymond's  Archiv,  1879,  supplement,  pp.  1-10). 
2,  10,  11.  12,  15,  20,  24,  25,  26,  27,  28. 
x.  Mode  of  Re-actions;  Analysis  of  Re-actions. 

32.  ORCHANSKY  (reported  by  Gad).    Du  Bois-Reymnnd's  Archiv,  1887,  p. 

369. 

10,  13,  15,  24. 

xi.  Effect  of  Disturbance,  Expectation,  Practice,  and  Fatigue. 

11,  12,  13,  34,  35,  49. 


MENTAL    PHENOMENA.  oil 


xii.  Distinction  betireen  S'DI.SW//  and  Motor. 

33.  LANGE.    Neue  Experimeute  Qber  den  Vorganjj  der  eiufarhm  Reaction 

auf  Sinneseindriicke  (Wundt's  Studien,  iv.  pp.  479-511;. 

34.  MARTIUS.       Die      muskuliire     Reaction     und     die     Aufmerksamkeit 

(Idem,  vi.  pp.  167-'-!17). 
35.  DWELSHAUVERS.      Untersuchungen    zur   Mechanik   der  activen   Auf 

merksainkeit  (Idem,  pp.  217-249). 
45. 

xni.  Individual  Variations:  Personal  Equations. 

36.  SANFORD.      Personal  Equation  (American   Journal  of  Psycltolor/y,  it. 
pp.  3-38.  271-298,  403-430.    Consult  bibliography  there  given  for  de- 
tailed references). 
15,38. 

xiv.  Abnormal  Variations. 

37.  DIETL  and  VINTSCHGAU.      Das    Verhalten  dor  pbysiologischen    Reac- 

tionszeit    unter   den    Einfluss  von  Morphium,   Caffee  und  Wein 
(Pfliiger's  Archiv,  xvi.  pp.  316-4061. 

38.  OBERSTEINER.    Experimental  Researches  on  Attention  (Brain,  i.  pp. 

439-453). 

39.  OBERSTEINER.     Ueber  eine  neue  einfache  Methode  zur  Bestimmung 

des    psychischen    Leitungsfahigkeit   des    Gehirns  Geisteskranker 
(Virchow's  Archiv,  lix.  pp.  427^58). 

40.  KRAEPELIN.  Ueber  die  Einwirkung  einiger  medicamentoser  Stoffe  auf 
die  Dauer  einfacher  psychischer  Vorgiinge  (Wundt's  Studien,  i. 
pp.  417^62,  573-605). 

41.  WARREN.    The  Effect  of  Pure  Alcohol    on  the  Re-  action  Time,  etc. 

(Journal  of  Physiology,  viii.  No.  6,  pp.  311-348). 
15,  26,  32. 

42.  STANLEY  HALL.    Re-action  Time  in  the  Hypnotic  State  (Mind,  viii.). 

43.  JAMES.     Re-action  Time  in  the  Hypnotic  Trance  (Proceedings  of  the 

American  Society  for  Psychical  Research,  i.  3,  pp.  246-248). 
xv.  Appliances  and  Methods. 

44.  SANFORD.    A  Simple  and  Inexpensive  Chronoscope  (American  Journal 

of  Psychology,  iii.  pp.  174-182). 
1,  11,  13,  14,  39,  41. 

C.  ADAPTIVE  RE-ACTIONS. 

(Distinction  and  Choice.) 
i.  General  and  Theoretical. 
45.  MUNSTERBERG.     Beitrage  zur  Experimentellen  Psychologic,  i.  pp.  64- 

188. 
1,  4,5,6,7,9.  10.  I:.'.  13,  4!). 

46.  KRIES.     Ueber  Uuterseheidungszeiten  (Vierteljahrsschrift  fiir  wissen- 

schaftliche  Philosophic,  xi.  pp.  1-23). 

47.  CATTELL.    Ueber  die  Zeit  der  Erkeunung  und  Benennung  von  Schrift- 

zeichen,  Bildern,  und  Parbeu  i  M"ini</f  .s  Studien,  ii.  4,  pp.  635-650). 
.    n.  Effect  of  Number  of  Distinctions  and  of  Choices. 

48.  TIGERSTEDT  and  BERGQVIST.     Zur  Kenntniss  der  Apperceptionsdauer 

zusammengesetzter    Gesichtsvorstellungen   (Zeitschrift  fiir  Bio- 
logic. xix.  pp.  5-44). 

49.  TISCHER.      Ueber  die    Unterscheiduug    von    Schallstiirken   (Wundr* 

Studien,  i.  pp.  495-542). 
50.  MERKEL.    Die  zeitlichen  Verhaltnisse  der  Willcusthatigkeit  (Idem.  ii. 

PP.  73-127). 
1,  10,  13,  14,34. 


CO  THE   TIME-RELATIONS   OF   MENTAL   PHENOMENA. 

in.  Specific  Nature  of  Impression. 

1,  4,  5,  6,  7,  9,  10,  11,  12,  13,  15,  27,  29,  30. 

iv.  Foreknowledge  of  Subject. 

1,  13,  54. 

v.  Mode  of  Re-action:  Association  of  Stimulus  and  Movement. 
13,  34. 

vi.  Overlapping  of  Mental  Processes:  Sensory  and  Motor  Re-actions. 
13,  44,  35,  52. 

51.  PAULHAN.    Revue  Scientifique,  1887. 

VII.  Practice  and  Fatigue. 

52.  BERGER.      Ueber   den    Einfluss   der   Uebung   auf   geistige   Processe 

(WundVs  Studien,  v.  pp.  170-178). 
1,  12,  13,  27,  50. 

vni.  Miscellaneous  and  Individual  Variations. 
1,  9,  13,  39,  40,  49,  50. 

D.  ASSOCIATION  TIMES. 

i.  General. 

53.  CATTELL.     Experiments  on  the  Association  ot   Ideas  (Mind,  xii.  pp. 

68-74). 
1,  4,  45. 
II.  Questions  limited  to  a  Single  Answer,  involving  Acts  of  Memory. 

54.  VINTSCHGAU.     Die  physiologische  Zeit  einer  Kopfmultiplication  vou 

zwei  einziffrigen  Zahleii  (Pliiger's  Archiv,  xxxvii.  pp  187-202). 

45,  53. 
in.  Questions  limited  to  a  Single  Answer,  involving  Comparison. 

45,  53. 
iv.  Questions  admitting  of  Various  Answers. 

45,  53,  56. 

v.  Unrestricted  Associations. 

55.  CATTELL  and  BRYANT.    Mental  Association  investigated  by  Experiment 

(Mind,  xiv.  pp.  230-250). 

56.  TRAUTSCHOLDT.     Experimentelle   Untersuchungen  iiber  die  Associa- 
tion der  Vorstellungen  (  Wundfa  Studien,  i.  pp.  213-250). 
1,  14,  45. 
57.  GALTON.    Inquiries  into  Human  Faculty,  pp.  182-203. 


Fact  and  Theory  Papers 

THE  SUPPRESSION  OF  CONSUMPTION. 

By  GODFREY  W.  HAMBLETON,  M.D.  12°.  40c. 

"The  inestimable  importance  of  the  subject,  the 
eminence  of  the  author  and  the  novelty  of  his  work 
all  combine  to  render  the  little  treatise  worthy  of 
special  consideration.  .  .  .  We  heartily  com- 
mend Dr.  Hambleton's  booklet  and  wish  there  were 
more  such  works."— Editorial,  Boston  Daily  Adver- 
tiser. 

THE  SOCIETY  AND  THE  "FAD." 

By  APPLETON  MORGAN,  Esq.    12°.     20  cents. 

In  this  book  Mr.  Morgan,  who  is  president  of  the 
New  York  Shakespeare  Society,  sets  forth  what  he 
believes  to  be  the  true  function  of  a  Shakespeare 
Society,  which  in  many  respects  he  makes  essen- 
tially scientific. 

PROTOPLASM  AND  LIFE. 

By    C.    F.     Cox.      12°.      75   cents. 

The  author  of  this  book  was  for  some  years  presi- 
dent of  the  New  York  Microscopical  Society,  and  in 
this  volume  he  sets  forth  his  views  on  the  spontane- 
ous generation  theory  and  its  relation  to  the  general 
theory  of  evolution,  and  on  protoplasm  a,nd  the  cell 
doctrine. 

THE  TORNADO. 

By  H.  A.  HAZEN,  12°.     $1. 

Professor  Hazen  is  one  of  the  prominent  meteor- 
ologists connected  with  the  United  States  Signal 
Office.  In  this  work  he  reviews  our  present  infor- 
mation as  to  tornadoes,  severely  criticising  some  of 
the  opinions  held  in  regard  to  them  up  to  this  time. 
No  one  has  given  a  more  careful  study  to  these  de- 
structive storms  than  has  Professor  Hazen,  and  his 
book  will  prove  a  decided  contribution  to  the 
world's  knowledge. 


N.  D.  C.  HODGES,  47  Lafayette  PL,  New  York 


Fact  and  Theory  Papers 


THE  CHEROKEES  IN  PRE-COLUMBIAN  TIMES 

By  CYRUS  THOMAS.     12°.     $1. 

Dr.  Thomas  in  this  work  will  reverse  the  usual 
method  of  dealing  with  prehistoric  subjects  ;  that  is 
to  say,  he  will  commence  with  the  earliest  recorded 
history  of  the  tribe  as  a  basis  and  trace  the  chain 
back  step  by  step  by  the  light  of  the  mounds,  tradi- 
tions, and  other  evidence,  as  far  as  possible.  He 
has  already  presented  to  the  public  some  reasons 
for  believing  the  Cherokees  were  mound-builders, 
but  additional  evidence  bearing  on  the  subject  has 
been  obtained.  A  more  careful  study  of  the  Dela- 
ware tradition  respecting  the  Tallegwi  satisfies  him 
that  we  have  in  the  Bark  Record  (Walam  Olurn) 
itself  proof  that  they  were  Cherokees.  He  thinks 
the  mounds  enable  us  to  trace  back  their  line  of 
migration  even  beyond  their  residence  in  Ohio  to 
the  western  bank  of  the  Mississippi.  The  object  is 
therefore  threefold:  1.  An  illustration  of  the  re- 
verse method  of  dealing  with  prehistoric  subjects  ; 
2.  Incidental  proof  that  some  of  the  Indians  were 
mound-builders  ;  3.  A  study  of  a  single  tribe  in  the 
light  of  the  mound  testimony.  This  work  will  be  an 
important  contribution  to  the  literature  of  the  Co- 
lumbian discovery  which  will  doubtless  appear 
during  the  coming  two  years. 


THE  WINNIPEG  COUNTRY; 

OR, 

ROUGHING  IT  WITH  AN  ECLIPSE  PARTY. 

BY 
A.   KOt  HESTER  KEM,O\\  . 

(S.  H.  SCUDDER.) 

With    thirty-two    Illustrations    and    a  Map. 
12°.     $1.50. 

"The  story  is  a  piquant,  good  humored,  entertain- 
ing narrative  of  a  canoe  voyage  A  neater,  prettier 
book  is  seldom  seen."— Literary  World. 

"This  is  a  sprightly  narrative  of  personal  inci- 
dent. The  book  will  be  a  pleasant  reminder  to 
many  of  rough  experiences  on  a  frontier  which  is 
rapidly  receding."— Boston  Transcript. 

"  The  picture  of  our  desolate  North-western  terri- 
tory twenty-five  years  ago,  in  contrast  with  its 
civilized  aspect  to-day,  and  the  pleasant  features  of 
the  writer's  style,  constitute  the  claims  of  bis  little 
book  to  present  attention.1'—  The  Dial. 


N,  B.  C.  HODGES,  47  Lafayette  PL,  New  York. 


SCIENCE. 

I3ST    1S83_ 


A  WEEKLY  journal  devoted  to  recording  the  progress  of  science 
and  the  arts.  Among  recent  contributors  may  be  named:  —  A. 
Melville  Bell,  Joseph  Jastrow,  J.  H.  Raymond,  M.D.,  G.  Stan- 
ley Hall,  R  H.  Thurston,  H.  T.  Cresson,  H.  B.  Bashore,  H.  E. 
Stockbridge,  Lieut.  Bradley  A.  Fiske,  John  T.  Stoddard,  Charles 
S.  Minot,  Jacques  W.  Redway,  Nelson  W.  Perry,  T.  Berry  Smith, 
Robert  H.  Lamborn,  Gardiner  G.  Hubbard.  Edgar  Richards, 
H.  A.  Hazen,  Cyrus  Thomas,  T.  C.  Chamberlin,  A.  E.  Dolbear, 
W.  M.  Davis,  L.  W.  Ledyard,  John  C  Branner,  A.  T.  Drum- 
mond,  G.  Brown  Goode,  Burt  G.  Wilder,  Godfrey  W.  Hamble- 
ton,  M.D. 

Among  the  subjects  discussed  may  be  mentioned  :  —  The  Sup- 
pression of  Consumption,  Cookery  for  the  Poor,  Marine  Biological 
Laboratory,  Movements  in  Young  Children,  The  Phonograph  in 
the  Study  of  Indian  Languages,  Effigy  Mounds,  Lightning-Dis- 
charge, Sunspots  and  Tornadoes,  Stanley's  Explorations,  Pho- 
netics, The  Influence  of  Baking-Powder  Residues  on  Digestion, 
Unconscious  Bias  in  Walking,  Jade,  Clark  University,  The 
Influenza,  Hallucinations  in  Alcoholism. 

News  is  promptly  given  of  all  new  books  of  importance  not 
fiction. 

- 

$3.50  per   Year  ;  $i,    Trial  Subscription  for  Four  Months. 


N.  D.  C.  HODGES, 

47  Lafayette  P1<u*e,      -  New  York. 


I